THE-5‘3 SIIAHTYLBR IlllillfllHIillllillfllillilllllllllIUUIH [Illlfllliil 3 1293 0157 LIBRARY Michigan State University This is to certify that the dissertation entitled THE RELATIONSHIP OF STUDENT PERCEPTIONS AND BEHAVIORS REGARDING PERSONAL SAFETY: A Comparative Study of Two Small, Private Colleges presented by Mary-Beth Ann COOper has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for Ph .D . degree in Educational Administration Date MS U i: an Aflimatiw Action/Equal Opportunity Institution 0-1277! _ 'n . 6 PLACE IN RETURN BOX to remove this checkout from your record. To AVOID FINES return on or bdoro date duo. DATE DUE DATE DUE DATE DUE THE RELATIONSHIP OF STUDENT PERCEPTIONS AND BEHAVIORS REGARDING PERSONAL SAFETY: A COMPARATIVE STUDY OF TWO SMALL, PRIVATE COLLEGES BY Mary-Beth Ann Cooper A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Educational Administration 1997 ABSTRACT THE RELATIONSHIP OF STUDENT PERCEPTIONS AND BEHAVIORS REGARDING PERSONAL SAFETY: A COMPARATIVE STUDY OF TWO SMALL, PRIVATE COLLEGES BY Mary-Beth Ann Cooper A prediction study design was used to describe the relationship between safety related beliefs, demographic and enabling variables and the intention to use safety precautionary strategies on two college campuses. In addition, this study examined the relationship between safety behavioral intentions and the use of personal safety devices. Residential students at two small, private colleges were surveyed regarding their perception of personal safety and their motivation to comply with recommended safety behavior. Seven hypotheses were tested using z-tests for percentages and analysis of variance for means. Multiple regression analysis was used for each of the dependent variables to determine those factors which have the greatest impact on predicting safety behavior intention. The major finding of this study is that it is possible to predict intended safety behavior and safety device usage by using a combination of safety belief, enabling and demographic variables. Dedicated in loving memory of John Raymond Buckley and Angela Kochan Buckley iii ACKNOWLEDGMENTS I would like to acknowledge and thank my advisor, Dr. Kathryn Moore, for her patience and guidance as I completed this dissertation. Her belief in me was instrumental as I persevered with this project. Many thanks to my role models in higher education who served as true educators and have inspired me through my early professional years. I offer my deep appreciation to Mr. David Butler, Dr. Robert Harris, Dr. William Pickett, Dr. Larry Roper and Mr. Brian McAree. Without their guidance and support, I would not have achieved my personal and professional goals. I wish to thank my brothers, Mark, Matthew, Peter, Daniel and Paul, for their continued love and support. And finally, I wish to acknowledge my husband, Dave Cooper, who demonstrated incredible patience through this process. Dave's sacrifice of time and disproportionate care of Cal during the last six months of the completion of this project, was commendable. You have my gratitude and love. iv TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES .......................................... vii LIST OF FIGURES ......................................... viii CHAPTER 1 Introduction and Statement of the Problem ................ 1 Purpose of the Study ..................................... 8 Significance of the Study ................................ 13 Limitations of the Study ................................. 13 Organization of the Study ................................ 13 CHAPTER 2 REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE Introduction ............................................. 15 Campus Crime ............................................. 16 Environmental Issues ..................................... 21 Students Perceptions of Personal Safety .................. 24 Health Belief Model ...................................... 26 Risk Behavior ............................................ 31 Summary .................................................. 37 CHAPTER 3 METHODOLOGY Target Population ........................................ 38 Instrumentation .......................................... 40 Data Collection .......................................... 41 Research Design .......................................... 43 Data Analysis ............................................ 46 Research Hypotheses ...................................... 47 Definition of Terms ...................................... 47 CHAPTER 4 FINDINGS Profile of Respondents ................................... 49 Enabling variables ....................................... 53 Safety Belief Variables .................................. 54 Safety Behavior Intention ................................ 66 Safety Device Usage ...................................... 71 College Crime Statistics ................................. 77 Summary of Statistical Hypotheses ........................ 79 CHAPTER 5 CONCLUSIONS Summary of Findings ...................................... 81 Implications for Practice ................................ 87 Limitations .............................................. 90 Recommendations for Future Research ...................... 92 TAPPENDICES Appendix A: Hazareth Survey .............................. 94 Appendix B: Nazareth Letter .............................. 101 Appendix C: Nazareth Return Postcard ..................... 102 Appendix D: Fisher Survey ................................ 103 Appendix E: Fisher Letter ................................ 110 Appendix F: Fisher Return Postcard ....................... 111 Appendix G: Reminder Postcard ............................ 112 Appendix H: Letter of Approval for Study ................. 113 Appendix I: Campus Survey Request ........................ 114 Appendix J: Response from Survey Author .................. 115 BIBLIOGRAPHY ............................................. 116 vi \D (D «our!» (HMO-J uh uh uh 03> bub uh #- rb huh-Db huh-ab H O .11 .12 .13 .14 .15 .16 LIST OF TABLES Frequency Distribution of Demographic Variables ..... 52 Enabling Variables, Z-Test for Percentages .......... 54 Safety Dimensions/Scales, Independent z-Tests for Percentages ..................................... 55 Safety Precautions Recorded by Fisher Students ...... 62 Safety Precautions Recorded by Nazareth Students....63 Ratings of Safety Precautions by Percent ........... 65 Safety Behavior Intention, Likelihood of Taking Specific Safety Precautions During the Month ........ 67 Safety Behavior Intention, z-Test for Percentages Comparison Groups: Gender & College ................. 68 Stepwise Multiple Regression on Safety Behavior Intention with Demographics ......................... 71 Safety Device Users Categorized by Demographic Variables, z-Test for Percentages ................... 72 Summary of Beeper Usage ............................. 73 Safety Belief Variables by Beeper Usage, z-Test for Percentages .............................. 74 Enabling Factors by Beeper Usage, z-Test for Percentages ..................................... 75 Safety Behavior Intention by Beeper Usage, Z-Test for Percentages .............................. 76 Stepwise Multiple Regression on Beeper Usage with Demographics ................................... 77 Campus Crime Statistics 1992-1996 By College ........ 78 vii LIST OF FIGURES Figure 3.1 Safety Belief Model II ......................... 44 Figure 3.2 Independent & Dependent Variables .............. 45 viii Chapter 1 INTRODUCTION AND STATEMENT OF THE PROBLEM Students who experience crime or its impact on their campus may be less successful in their educational pursuits and in their satisfaction with their collegiate experience. Komisar states that.“as institutions devoted to the life of the mind and the pursuit of truth, colleges and universities have an especially urgent claim on freedom from fear. Scholarly inquiry and transmission of knowledge can only go forward in the presence of order and in the absence of threat" (Komisar, 1986 in Kobasic, Smith & Zucker, 1988, p. 575). Incidents of campus crime have increased through the 1970's and early 1980's, reaching a plateau that is likely to continue to hold steady through the 90's (Smdth, 1989). In an attempt to require colleges to protect their students and employees from crime and violence, many victims and families of victims have turned to the court system for intervention. The trend of suing universities has grown significantly; in fact, the number of court cases brought against institutions of higher education has tripled in the last ten years (CaStelli, 1990). In addition, federal and 2 state governing bodies have enacted legislation geared to informing and protecting campus community members. Lenski states, "Colleges and universities should have keen interest in any examination of safety behavior which may enable them to respond more completely to the legislation of the 1990 Campus Security Act and which has any applicability to the prevention of crimes that consume national attention by current and prospective students and families" (Lenski, 1992, p. 4). The legal case behind the 1990 Campus Security Act was brought by Mr. and Mrs. Howard Clery against Lehigh University. The Clery's daughter was raped and murdered in her dorm room at Lehigh in 1986. In the settlement, Lehigh agreed to spend more than $1 million to install better lighting, alarm systems, to hire more security guards and to implement a shuttle system for students after dark (Hanchette, 1988). More than just the changes to the environment by Lehigh administrators, the Clerys sought assistance from the Pennsylvania Legislature to enact the College and University Information Act. This initial statute was modified and later developed into the Campus Security Act of 1990, which requires campuses to report campus crime and to publicize college safety policies. As recent federal and state legislation have mandated that college and university administrators review their current operating procedures regarding campus safety, 3 heightened awareness of the topic has resulted. It seems straightforward to simply respond to legislative mandates, but that does not provide a comprehensive approach to educating students about the realistic possibilities of campus crime. A more inclusive approach to understanding campus crime and its impact would be for college administrators to know critical information such as: student feelings regarding safety, their perceptions of safety on campus, and student reported behaviors related to personal safety. WW Campus residence halls and their surrounding areas, while seen as significant learning environments on campus, can often represent settings for potential crime at a college or university (College and University Law Enforcement Officers, 1985). Common approaches to dealing with safety education on college campuses have included strategies for avoiding crime, self-defense workshops, lengthy reports of crime statistics and reporting mechanisms for victims of campus crime. Although all these approaches have value, do campus educators and students understand why crime occurs, or to whom? It is imperative to learn how students perceive their personal safety on campus and how their behavior may increase or decrease the likelihood of them.becoming victims. When designing safety and security programs for 4 campuses, institutions need to know how to best advise students about personal safety on campus. It is a challenge to simultaneously increase students' awareness of safety risks associated with campus life and also empower students to take more responsibility for themselves. Lenski (1992) suggests that college administrators need to know more about students' beliefs, perceptions, attitudes, and motivations with regard to safety in order to design a comprehensive and successful approach to help students see the connection between their actions, the college environment and potential consequences of risky behavior. As colleges take the initiative to implement preventive measures, how does one know which measures will work? The research on effectiveness of safety interventions is extremely limdted. It is challenging to assess effectiveness of security programs because of the variety of variables that are difficult to control on college campuses. Yet, if the security measures are going to be put in place, it is important to learn more about the effects of interventions on college students. The goal of this project is to examine the effect of one type of a security intervention and how it impacts students' safety related behavior on a small campus. Recently the role of students' perceptions in understanding campus safety was addressed in a study funded through the Association of College and University Housing Officers-International (Lenski, 1992). The project focused 5 on the relationship between college students' beliefs about safety on campus, demographic and other modifying factors, and the intention to practice precautionary behavior. The researcher found that students who perceive safety precautions as inconveniences are less likely to take those precautions, while students who describe themselves as being more concerned about personal safety were more likely to plan on taking safety precautions, and students who were concerned about the ways crime victimization might affect their lives were more likely to demonstrate less risky behavior on campus (Lenski, 1992). The findings from this study are significant in evaluating current safety awareness programming and its impact on students' attitudes and beliefs about crime and victimization on campus. .College resident students, ages 18 to 24, are a group that is traditionally vulnerable to crime. Residence hall or community living is a new experience for most of these students and requires that they adjust to group living and develop a variety of skills in taking personal responsibility for protecting themselves, including their property (Wills, 1993). When crime (i.e. theft of personal belongings, violence, vandalism) does occur, many students are often unsure about what to do; they may even choose not to report criminal activity to the authorities. Often the crime itself is overwhelming and the process of reporting it prolongs their role as a victim" Beyond those students directly effected by crime, for the rest of the community a 6 feeling of invulnerability often prevails. Invulnerability is defined in terms of an incapability of being harmed, injured, or wounded (Turner, 1993). Dolcini (1993) notes that given the widespread acceptance of the idea that young adults hold exaggerated beliefs concerning their indestructibility, it is surprising that more research has not focused on risk-taking behaviors, including understanding the origins of perceived invulnerability (Dolcini et al.,1989). H 1" E 1' E H i J i S E | E J' E H i J In the 1950's, a project was set up by the US Public Health Service to explain the widespread failure of people to participate in programs to prevent or detect disease. The outcome of this project was the development of the Health Belief Model, which has become one of the most widely used and accepted psychological approaches to explaining health related behavior (Rosenstock, 1990). Basically the model is a theoretical approach to understanding "the forces that drive and inhibit behavior as well as the individual's environment and personal characteristics" (Becker et a1, 1977). Rosenstock (1990) states that in its most current form, the model suggests that individuals will take action to avoid or control health conditions after considering a number of factors which are present or available to them. Some of those factors suggested are: perceived susceptibility of contracting a health condition, perceived 7 seriousness of contracting an illness, perceived benefits of taking health action, elimination or minimization of perceived barriers to health action. Researchers have attempted to apply the health belief theory to the topic of adolescent sexual behavior. In one study (Hinston et al., 1990) teenagers' beliefs about AIDs and condom use were directly related to self-reported behavior of use of condoms. More specifically, "the greater the perceived susceptibility to AIDS, perceived severity of AIDs, and perceived effectiveness of latex condom, the greater their reporting of 'always using' condoms" (Turner, 1993). At the collegiate level, a study done in 1995 by Lenski, Meyers and Hunter used the Health Belief Model as a base to design The Safety Belief Model. The Safety Belief Model combines variables identified in the Health Belief Model, along with demographic variables and behavior variables related to personal safety. The model is comprised of eight safety belief scales, demographic characteristics and certain life experiences, which combined could predict the likelihood of following safety precautions. Examples of precautionary behavior include locking doors, using a college escort system, walking with a prearranged individual, walking in well lit areas, and reporting suspicious persons to security personnel (Lenski, Meyers, Hunter, 1996). 8 The eight scales, each of which is targeted to a set of beliefs and behavioral intent, are: 1. Motivation to Comply: referred to positive health incentives such as attentiveness to physical health and wellness. 2. Perceived Benefits: assessed beliefs that compliance with recommended safety precautions would reduce the threat of victimization and the degree to which the individual and the institution are empowered to improve personal safety. 3. Perceived Barriers: included questions to assess students' negative perceptions of the recommended safety behaviors, including peers' opinions of safety precautions and convenience of use. 4. Perceived Susceptibility: composed of items related to feelings of vulnerability to property and personal crime on campus. 5. Perceived Severity: contained items for subjective assessment of the effect of crime victimization on college, work, and personal life. 6. Perceived Threat: composed of the latter two scales. 7. Safety Beliefs: created by pooling all the other belief scales, the purpose of which was to assess the combined predictive power of the model. 8. Safety Behavior Intention: outcome variable computed from summed ratings of a person's likelihood to take the listed precautions. (Lenski et al, 1992) PURPOSE_QE_IHE_SIUDX The purpose of the present study was two-fold. In an attempt to understand the impact of a security intervention on students at a small campus, this researcher used the Campus Safety Survey to investigate its effect on the 9 students' behavior. The specific purposes of the study are listed below: 1). To test the usefulness of the Safety Belief Model by using the Campus Safety Survey in predicting students' behavioral intentions. Specifically, what variable or combination of belief, demographic, and/or enabling variables predict the safety behavior intentions of residential students at St. John Fisher College and Nazareth College? 2). To examine the relationship between use of personal safety devices and safety behavioral intentions of residential college students at two small, private colleges. RESEARCH_QHESIIQNS This study addressed the question of whether the Safety Belief Model is useful in predicting students' behavioral intentions and whether a relationship exists between safety behavioral intentions and personal safety devices. These basic issues were formed into a set of testable research questions: 1. Is there a statistically significant relationship between any of the safety belief variables and safety behavior intention? 2. Is there a statistically significant relationship between any of the demographic variables and safety behavior intention? 3. Is there a statistically significant relationship between any of the enabling variables and safety behavior intention? 4. .Are there significant differences between safety device users and non-users in any of the safety belief variables? 5. Are there significant differences between safety device users and non-users in any of the demographic variables? 10 6. Are there significant differences between safety device users and non-users in any of the enabling variables? 7. Are there significant differences between safety device users and non—users in safety behavior intention? INDEEENDENI_YARIABLES S E E J’ E H . I] : 1. Motivation to Comply with recommended behavior 2. Perceived Susceptibility to campus crime (part of Perceived Threat variable) 3. Perceived Severity of crime victimization (part of Perceived Threat variable) 4. Perceived Benefits of taking precautions 5. Perceived Barriers to taking precautions M 1.: . M . l] : 1. Demographic variables Gender, Race/Ethnicity, Age, Class standing, Length of time lived on campus, Self-reported grade point average 2. Enabling variables (prior victimization or sensitization to topic) 3. College Nazareth or St. John Fisher DE2ENDENTLIOUICOMEI_YARIABLES 1. Safety Behavior Intention, a score derived from intended use of a series of safety precautions 2. The likelihood of using a safety device beeper. METHODOLOGY The two institutions used in this study are similar in size (approximately 1600 students total, 800 residential 11 students on each site); both are small, liberal arts institutions founded in the Roman Catholic tradition, located in the suburban area of a mid-size Northeastern city. What is significantly different is that one of the campuses has recently installed a security system for members of its campus community. At one campus, faculty, staff and students may obtain a "Security Escort" beeper from the Campus Safety Department. The small hand held transmitter when activated sends a message to a central switchboard area on campus, displaying the location and identity of the individual in need of assistance. Security personnel respond to the geographic spot where the beeper has been activated. The adoption of this device has had a significant impact on the campus, in that over 65% of the population are participating in this personal safety system. The second campus does not have an advanced security system in place. Although approached by the same vendor of the described security system, the administration is skeptical of the effectiveness of such an intervention. The institutions in the study have similar educational programming targeted at increasing awareness of personal safety on campus, trained security officers who respond to criminal activity on campus, and campus personnel work to influence the quality of life within their campus communities. So, by focusing the study on the impact of this security beeper system, the value of the product in terms of 12 how it impacts its users and nonusers, was examined. To obtain the necessary data for the study, the Campus Safety Survey (Lenski, 1992) was administered to all resident students at St. John Fisher College and Nazareth College during the Fall Semester, 1996. Lists of subjects were provided by the residential life offices at both campuses. The self-administered questionnaire was mailed directly to each student along with a cover letter describing the study. A follow-up letter and second copy of the questionnaire was sent after two weeks to all non- respondents. Students were asked to return a separate postcard at the same time they returned their questionnaire. In the cover letter, the students were notified that returned postcards (along with questionnaire), were to be used in a drawing for a gift certificate for movie passes and a dinner at a local restaurant in close proximity to both campuses. W The research questions were investigated using the slightly modified Campus Safety Survey developed by Lenski (1992). The data was analyzed using descriptive statistics, multiple regression analysis, and analysis of variance. 13 SIGNIEIOANOE_OE_IHE_SIHDX The challenge presented to college administrators is to reduce legal exposure and assist campus community members to feel safe, and at the same time to recognize individual responsibility while on campus. It was this researcher's intent to assess residence hall students' perceptions of personal safety on college campuses and to investigate the variables which impact likelihood of using precautionary behavior. A significant focus of the study was to determine the impact of personal safety devices used on one of the Campuses in the sample. I' . l' E I] SI 3 1. Both institutions used in the study are small, private religiously affiliated colleges. The results of this study will be generalizable only to similar populations. 2. The length of the survey may result in a lower than desired return rate. 3. The use of behavioral intention as an outcome, rather than self-reported behavior makes the emphasis of this study prospective rather than retrospective. It is possible that the actual behavior of the respondents during the month following the administration of the instrument, may be different than their recorded intentions. Behavioral intention should not be confused with actual behavior(which this researcher is not including in this study). B . l' E I] SI 1 The study is organized into five chapters with the addition of appendixes. Chapter 2 is a review of the l4 literature relevant to influences on social behavior, campus safety research and the social-psychological framework of risk behavior. Chapter 3 describes the methodology and includes descriptions of the sample, variables, instrumentation, data collection and analysis procedures. The fourth chapter contains an analysis of the data. Chapter 5 presents a discussion of the findings, implications for Student Affairs practitioners, and recommendations for future research. Chapter 2 REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE Introduction In this chapter, a review of the literature related to campus safety is presented: (1) crime on college campuses, (2) environmental influences on human behavior, (3) Health Belief Model and preventative health behavior, and (4) Developmental Theory of Adolescent Reckless Behavior (Jessor, 1992). The first section provides an overview of campus crime and recent legislative measures. Recent statistics regarding crime victimization and factors associated with campus crime rates will be included. The second section focuses on the campus environment as an influence on behavior. Literature on campus design and residence hall environments is included in this section. Section three includes a review of the research on the Health Belief Model, of primary interest in this study because the instrument this researcher used is modeled after the Health Belief Model's assessment of attitudes, beliefs, perceptions and behavioral intent. The final category focuses on Jessor's social-psychological framework for the explanation of adolescent risk behavior. The exploration of risk 15 16 behavior and prevention/intervention will be of particular interest to this study of the relationship of student perceptions and behaviors regarding personal safety. W The responsibility of an institution of higher education for the safety of its students has been explored by various courts in recent years. A college's responsibility is illuminated by the following excerpt from the decision of Eiseman v. State of New York, (1985, p. 963): Students enroll in a college in the expectation that, not only will they be afforded the means to derive an education in an atmosphere conducive to the stimulation or thought and learning, but also that they will be permitted to do so in an environment reasonably free from risk of harm. A college is not expected to be a guarantor or insurer of the safety of its students, but obviously is expected to provide, in addition to an intellectual climate, a physical environment harmonious with the purposed of an institution of higher learning. To that end it employs a security force and establishes rules and regulations, breach of which can lead to suspension or expulsion. Since 1989, a series of high profile crimes took place on college campuses across the nation, making it a priority for college administrators whether they liked it or not. Maybe it was the Clery case, in which a 19 year old was raped, sodomized, and strangled in her dormitory room. Or the Hawelka case, where a student was raped and killed in full view of campus security officers who believed they were witnessing a young couple engaged in consensual sex. Or the l7 Pappas case, which found a financial-aid officer shot by a disgruntled man who had waited two hours to apply for financial assistance (Greene,1988). These are only three of the many cases of violent crimes which received significant press locally and nationally. As a result of the Clery murder, the victimfis parents decided to do something about what had happened to their family. Howard and Constance Clery were a major force behind a Pennsylvania law which required all colleges in the state to disclose crime statistics. The Clerys' goal was to have colleges and universities be compelled to abide by strict truth in advertising concerning crime and security on college campuses. The legislation, Pennsylgania_ggllggg_and W was signed into law in 1988. Two years later, one of the most significant and controversial bills was introduced and passed by the House of Representatives and the US Senate. The original draft was the Wat gf_1220* Revisions of this bill and clarifications of terms took place over the next four years. The legislation is officially known as Title II of the WIRED It requires colleges and universities to distribute to all current students and employees and applicants for enrollment or employment two types of information: (1) descriptions of policies related to campus security; and (2) statistics concerning specific types of crimes. Colleges were now 18 required to publish statistics in nine categories: incidents of murder, rape, robbery, aggravated assault, burglary, and motor—vehicle thefts, and arrests for liquor-law violations, drug-law violations, and weapons-law violations. Distribution must be made through appropriate publications and mailings on an annual basis (section 204 (a)). Although the intent of the legislation was to gain a more accurate picture of crime on campus, a debate continues over how to interpret the data submitted. College officials and in particular those who work with campus law enforcement, warn against drawing firm conclusions from the statistics provided by the reporting institutions (Lederman, 1995). Many feel that ambiguity still exists around who has to report what, that the definitions of the crimes are too broad and that simply, many colleges and universities fail to comply with the requirements listed in the Act of 1990. Of 796 campuses out of 5000 which responded to a survey done by The Chronicle of Higher Education in 1994, they reported the following: from 1993 to 1994 there was an increase of reports of robberies and aggravated assaults (+2.2%), a sharp increase in drug and weapons violations (up +34 and +11.2% respectively), a drop in number of burglaries and motor-vehicle thefts (down 5% and 3% respectively), and the total number of reported murders fell from 17 in 1992 to 15 in 1993. Rising numbers in the statistics could reflect increased enforcement, or an increase in criminal activity 19 on campuses, the reader is left not knowing the complete story (Ledeman, 1995). Many administrators do agree on one major point. Most feel great concern about the continuing increase in the number of violent crimes. USA_Tnday published in chronological order the violent crimes committed on campus during the fall term 1990 (Ordovensky, 1990). In a three month period, 33 assaults and/or murders (18 were murders) occurred on college campuses. The statistical analysis done by the flEA_IQda¥ staff indicated that every 3.4 minutes a larceny is committed on U.S. campuses, and every 1 hour 33 ‘minutes an auto theft is committed. Additionally, every 3 hours and 20 minutes an aggravated assault is committed and a rape is committed every 21 hours on America's campuses (Ordovensky, 1990). With the pressure on colleges to report crime reports, the consequences are complicated and varied. The issue of student privacy has surfaced at a number of institutions. Even though specific names are not a part of the reporting procedure, some parents and campus activists contend that the college community is entitled to know details about crimes, so they can be better educated and prepare themselves against similar crime. These concerns led to recent efforts in Pennsylvania to require Colleges to open security logs. However, unsealing security logs may implicate innocent parties or further victimize those 20 individuals who reported the crimes by divulging sensitive details to an entire community (Felsenthal,1994). Another area to discuss is the relationship between crime and alcohol use. Over 95% of the reported violent crimes on campus were either drug or alcohol related (noted in the introductory paragraphs of the Campus Security Act of 1990, provided by Uniform Crime Reports, 1989). In a publication entitled The Links Among.Drugs, Alcohol, and campus crime, (Bausell, Maloy, & Sherrill, 1989), the authors reported that students who commit crimes tend to be heavy drug and alcohol users. In addition, victims of crimes also tend to be heavier users of drugs and alcohol than their counterpart students who report that they have not been a victim of crime. Several studies support this relationship between drug and/or alcohol use and campus crime. Researchers at Towson State University's Center for the Study Prevention of Campus Violence found that violent crimes were associated with frequent drug and alcohol use by both perpetrators and victims. In three years of National Campus Violence Surveys completed by the Center, they reported response means demonstrating 52% of violent incidents involved alcohol or drugs (Towson State University, 1986, 1987, 1988). One of the revisions to the Act of 1990 was to require colleges and universities to report the number of arrests involving liquor law violations and drug abuse violations. 21 As campus crime receives national attention and reporting laws continue to focus on this critical issue, the relationship between alcohol and drug use and campus crime needs to continue to be explored. Enximnmentalesues The dormitory helped to create an atmosphere that invited frustration, argument, and crime. In the commons room of dormitory at South Carolina College in 1833, two students at the same moment grabbed for a plate of trout: only one of them survived the duel that ensued. Among the victims of the collegiate way (the notion that a curriculum, a library, a faculty and students are not enough to make a college) were the boy who died in a duel at Dickinson, the students who were shot at Miami in Ohio, the professor who was killed at the University of Virginia, the president of Oakland College in Mississippi who was stabbed to death by a student, the president and professor who were stoned at the University of Georgia, the student who was stabbed at Illinois College, the students who were stabbed and killed at the University of Missouri and the University of North Carolina. For their misfortune these victims of college life could thank the dormitory, the sometime house of incarceration and infamy that sustained the collegiate way (Rudolph, 1990, p. 97). Schneider (1977) pointed out that, historically, student housing had been seen as a means of controlling student behavior. She suggested that parents were comfortable with a restricted environment which was intended to protect the students from misbehaving. The reality is that colleges and universities with residence halls have many more opportunities for potential crime than their commuter counterparts (Fox & Hellman, 1985). Although staff 22 typically have the responsibility to encourage resident students to behave in ways to assure the safety of themselves, their personal property and their community members, students do not always comply with suggested safety measures. In 1993, the University of Maryland conducted a survey of safety and security programs in residence halls at fifty selected colleges and universities. Participating institutions were asked to identify the challenges they encounter with student behavior related to safety and security programs. The most often cited problems included: Propped entrance doors, tailgating or the practice of allowing non-residents to trail into a building without an official host, tampering with or vandalism to safety and security equipment and students' failure to lock their room door (Davidson, 1994). Doug Tuttle, Campus Safety Director at one of the responding institutions, agrees. He contends that most students, particularly freshmen, just aren't used to locking their bedroom door when they go to bed at night because they didn't do that at home....we have to teach them it's not just their bedroom door anymore: it's the only thing between them and the outside world (Tuttle, 1990, p. 37). It is the hope of housing administrators that a residential setting can be created where community members share expectations, responsibility, respect and consideration. In Maslow's (1954) hierarchy of needs, 23 meeting an individual's requirement to feel safe and secure is second only to meeting the individual's physiological needs. Residential environments need to satisfy the resident's need to feel safe and secure, otherwise little else will be accomplished (Simpson, 1994). When examining satisfaction of resident students needs, student perceptions and interpretations of the physical environment play a major role. While perceptions are subjective and particular to an individual, in the aggregate they theoretically become and define the culture in which the individual lives. This aggregate view represents the shared perception of a particular environment or setting (Pascarella and Terenzine, 1991). Pace, as a prominent writer on the field of perceptual models, has given significant attention to the study of collegiate environments and student perceptions of these environments. Pace illustrated the importance of the social climate of a college by showing relationship between social climate and student satisfaction. In Pace's view, there are behavioral and psychological dimensions of an environment. The psychological aspect is apparent in the beliefs, feelings and attitudes of members, while behaviors are inferred by actions and relationships among community members (Pace, 1984). Discrepancies between need for safety or feeling safe and high incidences of violence or crime, result in psychological conflict, strain and general dissatisfaction. 24 The result of this general dissatisfaction or negative feelings about the environment, can be demonstrated through attrition, vandalism, poor academic performance or isolation by community members (Pace, 1984). Campus ecology and milieu management seek to change the environment and diminish the negative conditions which influence students' experience on campus (Evans, 1983). While safety cannot be guaranteed, residential students should be assured that their safety is of the highest priority to college and university officials. Strange (1996) suggests to an extent that various campus physical features contribute to students' safety and security, their sense of belonging and familiarity with an institution, their ease of access and movement through its spaces, and their experience of membership in an educational community is the ultimate test of their design and purpose (p. 247). W313: Assessing students' perceptions of their environments is one step in understanding the complexities of the issue of safety on college campuses. The college environment exerts more influence on students' degree of satisfaction with the college experience than any characteristic an entering student brings with them to college (Astin, 1977). Therefore, what happens on the college campus does matter. Specifically, when an event happens on campus which violates the students' sense of security it is important to the 25 community and its members. In the article, "Preventing Violence on College Campuses," Roark (1987) identified some of the reasons for the vulnerability of college students as victims of violence. Typically, students are in a new setting, away from parental supervision and support, and among others who are also experimenting with new freedoms. Recognition of this factor as we deal with young adults, may lead to the implementation of strategies to reduce health risk and to participate in behaviors that enhance health. A major element present on college campus is the "it won't happen to me syndrome". In studies of health risks, the findings demonstrate the strong tendency that people tend to consider themselves less likely than others to be victims of disease (Harris & Guten, 1979; Kirscht, Haefner, Kegeles, & Rosenstock, 1966; Weinstein, 1980). An illustration of this was explored in a study conducted at the University of Iowa in 1974. When studying resident attitudes toward security in the residence halls, the phenomenon of students not seeing themselves as a potential victims of crime was dramatically demonstrated by the results of the research project. An initial survey of attitudes of resident students about security issues took place and just fifteen days later a coed was brutally murdered on campus. A follow-up survey (a replication of the original survey) was administered after the murder to determine the impact of the murder on student attitudes 26 toward residence hall security. The murderer was still at large and little information regarding the case was released to the community. The survey asked about the likelihood of violent crime in the halls and 90% of the respondents in both samples did not believe that a violent crime was likely to occur. Approximately 75% of the respondents said that they had not been concerned about a lack of security in their residence hall and there was no significant change in this response after the murder. The authors concluded that the murder of the coed did not significantly impact residents' attitudes toward security or alter their security related behavior (Miller, 1974). Heath_Bclie£_MDdel .The Health Belief Model was initially developed over thirty years ago by a group of social psychologists at the U.S. Public Health Service in an attempt to explain the widespread failure of people to participate in programs to prevent or to detect disease (Hochbaum, 1958; Rosenstock, 1960, 1966, 1974, 1990). Specifically, the Model grew out of Public Health Services staff members' frustration with the limited success of a number of programs sponsored by their organization in the 1950's. Since the model's development, it has been considered one of the most influential and widely used psychosocial approaches to explaining health- related behavior (Rosenstock, 1990). Kasl and Cobb define health behavior as any activity 27 undertaken by a person who believes himself to be healthy for the purpose of preventing disease or detecting disease in an asymptomatic stage (1966). This is in contrast to illness behavior or sick-role behavior, where a person who feels ill seeks a definition of the illness and/or treatment for the purpose of getting well (Rosenstock, 1974). The Health Belief Model investigates the belief that individuals will take action to ward off, to screen for, or to control ill-health conditions if they regard themselves as susceptible to the condition, if they believe it to have potentially serious consequences, if they believe that a course of action available to them would be beneficial in reducing their susceptibility to or the severity of the condition, and if they believe that the anticipated barriers to (or costs of) taking action are outweighed by its benefits (Rosenstock, 1990, p. 43). It is these combined levels of perceived susceptibility, severity, benefits and barriers that influence health-related behaviors. Additional influences to health-related behaviors include demographic, sociopsychological, and structural variables which affect individual perception, and how one chooses to act. The Health Belief Model attempts to describe motivation to act as a function of expected goal attainment (Maiman & Becker, 1974). The Health Belief Model in its current form consists of six combinations of independent variables. It portrays a 28 set of health beliefs and modifying factors as interacting together, predicting the likelihood of compliant behavior. The Model has its roots in a threat-avoidance logic, focusing on preventing or detecting serious disease. Rosenstock suggests that behavior to promote health is essentially the same as behavior to prevent disease and can be described by the variables listed in the model just as other preventive or screening behaviors can (Rosenstock, 1990). The Health Belief Model has been utilized as a theoretical foundation in a significant amount of research. Hochbaum studied more than 1200 adults in three cities in 1952, attempting to identify factors underlying the decision to obtain a chest X-ray for the detection of tuberculosis. In his interviews, he focused on beliefs the participants had concerning their susceptibility to tuberculosis and beliefs in the benefits of early detection. Perceived susceptibility included (1) the respondent's beliefs about whether tuberculosis was a real possibility in their case and (2) the extent to which he/she accepted the fact that one may have tuberculosis in the absence of all symptoms. For those participants who exhibited both of these beliefs, 82 percent had at least one voluntary chest X-ray during a specified period of time preceding the interview. On the other hand, of the group exhibiting neither of these beliefs, only 21 percent had obtained a voluntary X-ray during the same period. Hochbaum found that four out of the 29 five people who exhibited both beliefs took the predicted action, while four of five who accepted neither of the beliefs chose not to take the action. In support of the Model, Hochbaum appears to demonstrate the a particular action is a function of two interacting variables, perceived susceptibility and perceived benefits (Rosenstock, 1990). Another study applied the Health Belief Model in the context of obtaining influenza vaccinations by people thought to be at high risk for complications from the influenza infection (i.e. people over sixty-five, those individuals with chronic problems such as heart disease, diabetes of renal disease). The researchers , Larson, Olsen, Cole and Shortell, targeted a group of individuals in a family medical center following a flu epidemic. The participants completed a self-administered questionnaire to assess health beliefs and vaccination status. The response rate in this study was a significant 75 percent. What Larson and others, found was that “perceived severity of influenzafl “perceived susceptibility to influenzafl,“perceived efficacy of vaccinefi “perceived expensiveness of vaccine", “self-reported inconvenience” and “satisfaction" were all significantly correlated with vaccine behavior. The investigators concluded “this study has demonstrated that health beliefs regarding susceptibility, severity, [vaccine] efficacy are important factors in utilization of influenza vaccine” (p. 1211) . 30 Another experimental study, this one focusing on mothers' adherence to a diet for their obese children yielded further support for the model. This study included motivation, demographic and enabling variables. Multiple- item scales were used to assess each of the model's dimensions with the participants, prior to the mothers receiving a weight-reduction plan for their child. The researchers conducted a multiple regression analysis in which weight change measures were regressed against belief measures (Becker et al., 1977). Nine items from the motivation, susceptibility, severity and benefits categories accounted for approximately 49% of the variance in the children's weight change. The researchers concluded that health beliefs may be most important at the beginning of a regimen, but over time and in the introduction of factors (i.e. weight change, boredom with the regimen of the diet) other variables may play a significant role (Becker et al., 1977). Other research using the Health Belief Model has been done in studying preventative dental behavior, Tay-Sachs carrier status screening program, practice of breast self- examinations, attendance at screening programs for high- blood pressure to name a few. Summary reports from these studies provide support for the model. There are limitations to the model, such as the fact that some of the early research was done with small samples and were retrospective in nature. In addition, there has been 31 inconsistency in the questions used in all the studies, which points to difficulty in determining the overall value of the Model. However, the Health Belief Model still remains among the foremost models in health behavior prediction today (Glanz, Lewis, and Rimer, 1990). Lenski used the Health Belief Model as the prototype for the Safety Belief Model in her study in 1992. This researcher utilized the Safety Belief Model as a framework to examine precautionary behavior regarding personal safety behavior among residential college students. 8' 1 E 1 . Studies of specific health and safety hazards suggest that people tend to believe that they are invulnerable. Most people believe that misfortunes will more than likely strike others, not themselves. Specifically, people tend to believe that their own risks are below average (Harris and Guten, 1979; Kirscht et al., 1966; Robertson, 1977). In a study of 100 college students comparing their own chances of experiencing 45 different health and life threatening problems with the chances of their peers, they showed a significant optimistic bias for 34 of these hazards, consistently considering their own chances to be below average (Weinstein, 1982). Weinstein suggests that the realization that one's own risk is above average as a powerful motivator for change in behavior. Health promotion and safety campaigns need procedures and illustrations that 32 will lead people to recognize their own risk status, and eliminate the unrealistic optimism.demonstrated in this study (Weinstein, 1982). Many adolescents perceive themselves as being invulnerable to the risks and consequences of their own behavior. In a study focused on sexual risk-taking, researchers found that while adolescents usually acknowledge others suffering from the consequences of risk-taking behaviors, they perceive themselves as being somehow immune (Moore and Rosenthal, 1991). Despite having adequate knowledge of reproduction and contraception, pregnant adolescents still had difficulty believing that pregnancy “could happen to them”(Donnelly, 1990) . Robinson, 1988, investigated perceived invulnerability among teenage fathers, observed after concluding his research: Teenagers see themselves as immortal and invulnerable. They believe they are immune from such consequences as automobile accidents, death, -or becoming involved in unwanted pregnancies. Over and over again I have heard ‘I never really thought it would happen to her' when teenage fathers describe an unplanned pregnancy with their female partners. Such magical thinking helps teenagers--especially younger teenagers--to convince themselves that they are somehow special and exempt from the conditions under which others must abide. (p. 5) In an investigation of older adolescents, Burger and Burns (1988) found that low rates of contraceptive use was also attributable to perceived invulnerability. In a study of undergraduate college women, it was found that sexually 33 active females tended to see themselves as less likely than other students, other women their age, and women of childbearing age to become pregnant. Biglan et al.,(1990) revealed in an investigation that adolescents who engage in one type of sexual risk taking were also apt to engage in other high-risk sexual behaviors. They found that teenagers who have sex with multiple partners whom they did not know very well are also not likely to use condoms. Moreover, these forms of sexual risk-taking were also related to other health-risk behaviors, such as drinking alcohol, use of drugs or smoking cigarettes (Orr et al., 1991). Although significant research has been completed on adolescent behavior, very little research has been completed on perception of vulnerability and risk-taking behavior among traditional age college students. It is this researcher's intent to suggest risk behavior in college age students relates to their development as individuals in the late stages of adolescence. A number of college age students come to college under the legal age of eighteen. For all practical purposes, college administrators ‘treat’ their population as adults complete with all the responsibilities that come along with that status. Aside from biological age, not to mention personal development issues, many of today's college students may not be equipped emotionally to deal with the changes in their environment. One recent study of acquaintance rape prevention 34 programs at a rural community college found that perception of vulnerability can be affected by direct efforts. Gray (1990), Department Chair of Criminal Justice at Wor-Wic Tech Community College, suggests that educators must do more than disseminate information about crime on campus if their goal is to alter risk—taking behavior that may lead to students becoming victims of crime. Simply providing information about who is victimized and tips on avoiding being a victim will have little impact on students who do not feel vulnerable or who do not perceive the seriousness of the threat of victimization (Gray, 1990). Gray suggests that students must first think that they personally are at risk, before they will take the threat of crime seriously and make a commitment to reduce risk-taking behaviors. He found that by personalizing acquaintance rape prevention programs to include local data and examples of actual crimes which have occurred, perceptions of vulnerability and intent to avoid risk-taking behaviors were increased for the participants in his study. Students then see that others just like them, on their campus, in their town have been victims of crime. Thus, specific activities initiated by colleges and universities can influence student knowledge and behaviors and have the effect of reducing victimization. Many college students are entering the final stages of adolescence, which researchers have targeted as a life stage of high risk (Jessor, 1984). Jessor singles out adolescence 35 as "a time of relatively high risk in which risk-related learning takes place - learning new risk behaviors, of risk- prone personality dispositions, and of risk-enhancing opportunities in the environment" (Jessor, 1984, p.86). Jessor suggests that adolescence is period of change. Transitions occur from physical changes to social and psychological changes in a wide variety of domains and take place rapidly relative to its rate in nearly all other life stages. White (1975) suggests that the main developmental trends associated with late adolescence and early adulthood as: the stabilizing of ego identity; the freeing of personal relationships; the deepening of interests; the humanizing of values; and the expansion of caring. What White does not comment on is the focus on the variety of behaviors relevant to health. Jessor suggests several implications for health that are inherent in this life stage: (1) adolescence is a period in which a variety of behaviors relevant to health are initially learned and tried out, such as drug use or precocious sexual activity, (2) many of the psychosocial attributes that influence and regulate the occurrence of health-related behaviors - values, beliefs, attitudes, motivations, personal controls, self-concept, general lifestyle - are also acquired or consolidated during adolescence, (3) the changing environment of adolescence - peers come to play a greater role in this stage relative to the role of parent or other adults, there is greater access 36 to potentially health-compromising materials (drugs, alcohol, automobiles, metorcycles), (4) the pervasiveness and rapidity of personal and societal changes may require coping with feelings of inadequacy and expectations of failure and (5) the asynchrony of changes is likely to be stressful and problematic for health. Jessor suggests that based on these descriptors of this stage, that adolescence is a critical period for significant health-promoting intervention. In particular, that this is the time to discuss the management of personal responsibility for adolescents for their own health as well as the health and safety of others (Jessor, 1984). The primary causes of death and disability at this life stage are behavioral in origin. Forms of violence - traffic accidents, suicides, and homicides - constitute the leading cause of death among this age group. Jessor describes risk not only in terms of behavior, but also of personality attributes and environmental supports. For health educators the key concern should be to make adolescents aware that there are risks associated with many of the behaviors in which they engage. The choice of using risk behavior versus risk-taking behavior is suggested by Jessor to discuss the interrelatedness of many of the health compromising behaviors already suggested earlier. He suggests a “web of causation” comprised of five domains: social environment, perceived environment, personality, biology/genetics and other behavior. Risk behavior crosses over the different 37 domains and elements of the web of causation ought to be of great importance to the design of intervention efforts. Intervention programs should highlight the linkages between the adolescent problem behaviors, not attempt to focus on one issue at a time, since research shows that risk behaviors often occur simultaneously. Successful interventions should be oriented to healthy lifestyle choices by providing specific knowledge and developing attitudes and skills necessary to deal with the challenges of the environment and personal choices(Jessor, 1991). W In this chapter, an effort has been made to look at campus crime, the physical environment of the college campus and two theoretical approaches to examine human behavior. The Health Belief Model was reviewed as an important framework for the Safety Belief Model, used in this study. Campus crime and issues of personal safety have not been the focus of much research in higher education. The angle of examining college student behavior as it relates to the later stages of adolescence is a promising new way to view college student issues. Chapter 3 METHODOLOGY This study examined the relationship between safety belief variables, modifying variables and intention to use precautionary safety strategies. In addition, this study examines the relationship between personal safety devices and safety perceptions and behaviors of residential college students at two small, private colleges. This chapter will describe the sample, the instrument, methods of data collection, and how the data were analyzed. The instrumentation section will relate questionnaire items with the dependent and independent variables. W The population included all the residential students at two nearby private colleges located in the suburban area of a midesize Northeastern city. The two institutions, Nazareth College and St. John Fisher College are small, liberal arts colleges founded in the Roman Catholic tradition. Located a mile away from each other, the institutions have positive relationships with each other and the surrounding community of Rochester, New York. 38 39 Nazaretthllege Founded in 1924, Nazareth College has an undergraduate population of 1375 students. During the Fall Semester, 1996, 62% of these students reside on campus, in five separate residence halls. Of the 854 resident population, 75% are female and 25% are male. An underground tunnel system connecting many of the halls to central campus buildings provides convenience and protection against the elements of the often harsh climate of upstate New York. Since 1993, the Campus Safety Department of Nazareth College has provided a "Security Escort" beeper to all of their full time faculty, staff and students. The Security Escort beeper is a small hand held transmitter, which many of the users attach to a key ring for convenience. When activated by the user, the system instantly communicates the identity and location of the user to a central console located in the Campus Safety Office. This is accomplished through a network of radio receivers that are strategically located throughout the grounds and buildings of the Nazareth Campus. There is no charge to those who choose to obtain a beeper and participate in this personal safety system. During the Fall Semester, 1996, 55% of the resident population were enrolled with the Campus Safety Office as users of this personal safety system. 40 SL_John_Eisher_Ccllece Founded in 1948, St. John Fisher College has an undergraduate population of 1485 students. During the Fall Semester, 1996, 45% of these students resided in six residence halls on campus. Of the 674 resident population, 54% are female and 46% are male. Similar to Nazareth College, both campus life staffs include trained security officers who respond to criminal activity on campus, and who are responsible for educational programming on safety issues for campus members. This campus, however, does not have an advanced security system in place. W In this study, an attempt was made to survey all residential students at the two colleges. Students were contacted from lists provided by the Residential Life Offices at both campuses. Students in the survey were enrolled during the Fall Semester, 1996. Instnmentation Instrumentation for the study consisted of a questionnaire developed by Lenski(1992) titled The Campus Safety Survey. Her questionnaire was developed using questions from prior Health Belief Model research (from Maiman, Becker, Kirscht, Haefner & Drachman, 1977: Rosenstock, 1974b reported in Lenski, 1992). Likert scales were used in the instrument because they demonstrated 41 substantial convergent reliability when used in the Health Belief Model (Cummings, Jette & Rosenstock, 1978). The Campus Safety Survey was slightly modified to include questions regarding personal safety device use for both campus resident populations. Additional questions regarding the personal safety device were asked only of Nazareth College students. This author also tailored the demographic information so it was applicable to this population and environment. In April, 1996, a panel of seven resident students from each institution had an opportunity to examine the instrument for applicability, language and to determine the length of time needed to complete the instrument. Slight modifications were made to the language of two questions relating to beeper use as a result of this exchange. DataJollection Permission to conduct the study was requested and granted from the residential life directors at both institutions involved in the study. The author met with the resident advisors at both institutions two weeks prior to the questionnaires being distributed to explain the study and the importance of subject participation. Since all resident students were involved in the study, the researcher estimated that four weeks into the semester was adequate time for new students to acclimate and set patterns of behavior within the context of the college 42 environment. On September 30, 1996, all resident students at both colleges received a cover letter describing the study, a Campus Safety Survey, an addressed return envelope, and a postcard for the respondent to participate in the incentive program developed by the researcher (Appendices A,B,C,D,E,F and G). The cover letter (Appendices A and D) was signed by the director of Residential Life at Nazareth College and the researcher employed by St. John Fisher College. It explained the voluntary and anonymous nature of the study, emphasized the importance of a high participation rate, and provided a phone number and name of someone respondents could contact with questions regarding the study. In addition, the incentive program offered by the researcher, "Dinner and A Show" was explained in full detail. A follow-up postcard (Appendix G) was distributed to all resident students at both campuses on October 4, 1996. It reminded them to turn in their survey and to enter the "Dinner and A Show" drawing. They were also told that they could obtain another survey from their resident advisor if they had misplaced their original one. To increase the return rate, the researcher offered the Opportunity for respondents to participate in a drawing for a gift certificate to a local restaurant and two free movie passes. The "Dinner and a Show" program was only available for those who returned their postcards and completed surveys 43 by the October 7, 1996 deadline. Three winners were selected from each institution and their gift certificates and movie passes were delivered to them on October 14, 1996. Of the 1528 mailed questionnaires, 1003 were returned. Of those returned, 997 questionnaires were useable. The overall response rate for the study was 65%. Reseamhflsign Variables drawn from the Health Belief Model and campus crime literature were formulated into the Safety Belief Model I by Tammy Lenski in 1992. In the present study, Lenski's model was modified into Safety Belief Model II (see Figure 3.1). As with Lenski's Model, the safety belief variables and the safety behavior intention variable were set up as scales, since the responses to the questions in the survey targeting beliefs and intentions were ultimately summed to a score. Figure 3.2 lists the survey items that comprised each of the independent and dependent variables used in the analysis. As stated earlier in this chapter, the purpose of the study was to test the usefulness of the Safety Belief Model in predicting students' behavioral intentions. Another goal of the study was to examine the relationship between personal safety devices and safety related behaviors. AnDnmiLx The respondents were assured of complete anonymity. Each respondent was instructed to return their completed Eugen". 45 Frame 3.2 Way 8:54 Variables 0 Motivation to comply with recommended behavior W o Perceived susceptibility to campus crime Aseessedlikelihoodd’heeolningaptopertycsinevictin Assessedlikelihoodofbeeolningavioletnctimevietin Amusedfihelihoodofbecununsaviolentaimevicdneonpetedmothuaudsnu Feelingsot'nfietywhileoncenpmdmingthehy Feeliupsot‘safetywhileonanpusatnipnt Feelinpofsaietyinthetesideneehell Opiniouofownneedtotab-ietyplm o Faeeivedseverityol'crilnevieti-hation Deyeedworrydmnvictituimou mommammmm O Pereeivedbeneiitsoftakingprecautions Perceivedwilityoflnstinniontoredueevictimintion Peseeiveddailitydselftoreducevictimimion Amudtilitytodeiendseif Speafic‘ knowledgeaboutsefietypreeemous' Beliet'thet . in l .. . . El'fthatplm ”0". ma! I ucnmzan. . on F I' depeeificpreeennmons . .uciumm. .on O Perceivedbarriesstotakingprecautions Peers‘opiuionsd'safietyptecautions Oonvenienced’safetyprecamions mmmwmmmmmwmm:m “MMCaflinafiiendtowukwithaMdutJQCaflingeeoonsetvieeanet Micheaoppednmuingfiordnnight(d)Avoidiugpoorlyfitp¢hs.(e)lbckin¢ dommomatnight.(t)Lockingdommomwindowa®Clodngpmppedmior m¢)Amngingfafitndmsinfamifiarnurounding(i)Avonfinghevmpuues memmmflydmm Mm Variables o Demographicvarhbles e Enablingvariahles A! Priormdanceatctinle- Classstandin; mud-dime Lenahottitueoaaupus worm - Colese W A. Likelihood of using morn-ended Safety Precautions BeluviorlmScote B. likelihoodot’asiugpersoaalsafetybeeper 46 survey separately from their return postcard. Follow-up reminders were sent to all respondents, not knowing who had turned in their completed surveys or postcards for the drawing. No coding system for identification was used on any of the questionnaires or return postcards. SI I' I' J E J . Data from the survey were tabulated for the overall sample, as well as broken down by college (Nazareth, St. John Fisher), and a host of demographic and behavioral variables including gender, class (e.g. freshman, sophomore, junior, and senior), number of semesters residing on campus, use of Escort protection system, perceived vulnerability and safety awareness level. Differences between segments were evaluated for significance using z-tests for percentages, and analysis of variance for means. Differences between segments that had a probability of occurrence of less than .05 based on chance alone were considered statistically significant. Multiple regression analysis was used to identify those factors which have the greatest impact on predicting safety behavior intention and the use of a safety intervention, such as the Eacort system” A.mmltiple regression analysis provides an equation describing a dependent variable (e.g. safety behavior intention, use of a beeper) as a function of student responses on various attributes (the independent 47 variables). The weights assigned to the various attributes in the equation (so-called beta weights) can be interpreted as measures of relative importance of those attributes in predicting responses on the dependent variables. As Lenski did in her study, multiple regression was used to test the null hypothesis and also tested the Safety Belief Model by providing information on the strength of the scales, variables and the overall predictive power of the model. A regression analysis was run using the scales, demographic variables and enabling variables. Hypotheses 1. There will be no significant differences between any of the safety belief variables and safety behavior intention. 2. There will be no significant differences between any of the demographic variables and safety behavior intention. 3. There will be no significant differences between any of the enabling variables and safety behavior intention. 4. Safety device users will report higher scores on all of the safety belief variables. 5. There will be no significant differences for safety device users when categorized by demographic variables. 6. Safety device users will report higher scores on the enabling variables. 7. Safety device users will report higher scores on safety behavior intention. E E' . . E I . A student who lives in a residence hall(dorm) at St. John Fisher College or Nazareth College 48 Belief. A cognitive link between an object and what a person knows about the object (Fishbein & Ajzen, 1975) Behayicr_1ntenticn. An expressed indication of the probability a person will perform a specific behavior Lenski, 1992) Sa£n£¥_Behaxinr. Any activity undertaken for the purpose of preventing or reducing susceptibility to personal or property crime (Lenski, 1992) YinlenD_Peraonal_Crime. A violent crime committed against a person; includes murder, sex offenses, robbery, and aggravated assault. . A crime of property; includes burglary larceny-theft, auto theft, arson, and destruction of property. . A small, hand held transmitter when activated sends an electronic signal to Campus Security Personnel offered to resident students at Nazareth College. CHAPTER 4 FINDINGS Introduction Results of the data analysis are presented in this chapter. The survey respondents are described by the demographic and enabling variables reported. Questionnaire data, as it relates to the safety belief scales and the use of the beeper will be presented. Wars The sample represented two subgroups, resident students from Nazareth College and resident students from St. John Fisher College. D 1' I E I' St. John Fisher surveys comprised 473 of the 997 usable surveys. Forty percent of the Fisher respondents were male and sixty percent were female. Similar to the existing gender breakdown for the College cited in Chapter III, Fisher's resident population is 46% male and 54% female. Nazareth surveys comprised 524 of the 997 usable surveys. Twenty-nine percent of the Nazareth respondents were male and 71% were female. Again, this sample is similar to the 49 50 Nazareth resident population, which is 75% female and 25% male. Class representation in the sample for both institutions was very similar. For Fisher, the respondents were 37% freshman, 26% sophomore, 20% junior and 15% senior (2% did not report their class standing). For Nazareth, the respondents were 37% freshman, 24% sophomore, 20% junior, and 19% senior. Eighty-six percent of the Fisher respondents described themselves as White, 4% described themselves as Black or African American, 2.5% as Asian, Asian American, or Pacific Islander, 2% as Hispanic and less than 1% for each of the following categories: American Indian, Mexican American and Puerto Rican. The remaining 2% answered this question by describing themselves as “Other”. Ninety-one percent of the Nazareth respondents described themselves as White and less than 1% each for the following ethnicity groups: Asian, Asian American or Pacific Islander, American Indian, Puerto Rican, Black or African American, Mexican American or Chicano and Hispanic. The remaining 4% of the Nazareth sample described themselves as “Other”. The mean age of respondents for Fisher was 19.3 years with 81% reporting they were under 21 and 19% reporting they were 21 or over. The mean age of Nazareth respondents was also 19.3 years. The same age breakdown existed for the Nazareth respondents, 81% reported being under 21 and 19% 51 reported being 21 or over. When asked to respond to how many semesters they lived on campus, 41% of Fisher students stated less than one semester, 8% said one to two semesters, 25% stated three to four semesters and 25% reported five or more semesters. Nazareth respondents again are similar on this demographic variable. Forty-one percent reported less than one semester, 7% one to two semesters, 23% stated three to four semesters and 28% reported five or more semesters on campus. Both college samples spent similar amounts of time on campus in the residential environment. The Fisher sample self-reported their grade point average in the following breakdown: 16% at 3.5 - 4.0, 74% between 2.5 - 3.5 and 9% less than 2.5. The Nazareth sample reported their grade point average in the following manner: 25% at 3.5 - 4.0, 68% between 2.5 — 3.5 and 4% less than 2.5. Seventeen Nazareth respondents chose not to respond to this question. The mean grade point average was 3.2 for the Nazareth sample and 3.06 for the Fisher sample. Table 4.1 provides a frequency distribution of demographic variables for the Fisher and Nazareth respondents. 52 4.1 UINCY DISTRIBUTION OF DEMOGRAPHIC VARIABLES De-epaplie Variable Fisher I Fisha' 56 Nausea: f Nazareth '/o Gouda Male 188 39.7 109 20.8 Full: 280 59.2 411 78.4 ' ' 5 1.0 4 .8 Total 473 100.0 524 100 RaeelEthieity Wlie 406 85.8 477 91.0 Black or Afiie-I Am 20 4.2 2 .4 Am Asia Alain or 12 2.5 4 .7 Pacific Isl-h Lena / South / Canal 10 2.1 1 .2 Am - Am ink a Name 4 .8 3 .6 m Pub lit. 3 .6 3 .6 Maia: Amine or Cine.» 1 .2 1 .2 7 15 20 3.8 10 2.1 13 2.5 Tall 473 100.0 524 100.0 MB 18 or b 165 34.9 187 35.7 19 128 27.1 128 24.4 20 84 17.7 108 20.6 21+ 88 18.6 96 18.3 ' 8 1.7 5 .9 TM 473 100.0 524 100.0 Clan Shh Fm 173 36.6 191 36.4 Me 124 26.2 126 24.0 Ju'or 93 19.7 106 20.2 Sdtl 71 15.0 98 18.7 Mich; 10 2.1 3 .6 Total 473 100.0 524 100.0 Sm a Can:- < 1 194 41.0 212 40.4 1 - 2 36 7.6 39 7.4 3 - 4 119 25.1 124 23.7 5 + 119 25.1 146 27.9 ' 5 1.0 3 .6 Trial 473 100.0 524 100.0 Gnfi Poi! A” Una 2.5 41 8.7 20 3.8 2.5 - 3.0 158 33.4 142 27.1 3.0 ~ 3.5 190 402 215 41.0 3.5 - 4.0 75 15.8 130 24.8 ' ' 9 1.9 17 3.2 Ton! 173 100.0 524 100.0 53 E 11' M . 11 I E |' Another dimension of the Modifying Variables is what Lenski labeled Enabling Variables. Enabling variables include items concerning property crime victimization and violent personal crime experience. Table 4.2 illustrates responses from the sample for enabling variable items. As the table shows, Fisher students were somewhat more likely to report being a victim of a property crime (33% vs 25% of Nazareth students), although both segments were similar in terms of their experiences with violent crime (14% and 12% respectively). Nazareth students, however, were twice as likely to report attendance at safety awareness programs (49% vs 24% of Fisher students). When the expert panel of seven students from each college initially reviewed this question for the researcher, most of the Fisher students had initial difficulty with this question. After further investigation into the classes and workshops offered by both communities, this researcher learned that Nazareth entitles their self-defense workshops as “self-defense classes” and offers these free each semester, whereas Fisher offers a Tai Kwon Do class through the physical education department only during the fall semester for a fee. In addition, because of the Security EScort System in place at Nazareth, new student orientation programs spend a significant time (approximately two hours during the first day students are on campus) discussing the 54 logistics of the beeper system. These two considerations may account for the large difference in the samples. IUNe42 ENABLE“:VMJUABLESJDTESTFONHHHNJUTDNGES Enfluhu‘hwhbkdums Naumdh Fkbu' Have been a victim of property crime , 25%‘ 33%‘ Have been a victim of a violent personal crime 12% 14% Has attended crime mess or self-defense 49%“ 24%" endow») ‘p < .05 S E | E J' E M . 1] The Safety Belief Variables were scored and divided into'three ranges: high, mid-range and low. Table 4.3 illustrates the results of independent z-tests for percentages between the colleges. A discussion of statistically significant differences among the various demographic variables within the scales follows Table 4.3. 55 13Me43 SAFETYIHNflHHHONSEKMJISMDHHHHBHMDTTZflflfiflsIKHRFERCENTAGIS \hwhbk INuumuh thu' hflnhuhmnoCbmpw High Score 32%" 21%" Mid-range Score 41%‘ 33%" meSane 27%! 46%‘ PoodwdSqumbmw' HmmSone zen 28% hfidnmpeSane ERG «#06 meSone 3m% 2&% .HuomndSmndq' High Score 33% 31% hfidnmgeSane ZZK 19% LowSaxe 49% son HuummdBaufiu High Score 30%‘ 24%‘ hfidmuchune 42% 3m% meSane 28%! BEK' Euamdeufias High Score 28%"' 38%" Mid-range Score 35% 33% meSane 3T%‘ 29%‘ ’p < .05. H I' . 3 J . 1 E l l E 1 . The Motivation to Comply Scale is a significant variable because it includes the importance of general health interest in contributing to practicing safe behavior. Motivation to comply refers to positive health incentives such as attentiveness to physical health and wellness. In the overall scale, 32% of Nazareth students scored in the high range for motivation to comply compared to 21% reported by Fisher, a difference which was 56 statistically significant (see table 4.3). The Nazareth sample also showed a significant difference (p<.05) at the average and low levels, with Nazareth students continuing to report at a higher level for motivation and interest in practicing safe behaviors than Fisher students. Fisher respondents (46%) scored in the low range for practicing safe behavior compared to Nazareth response of 27% in the low range score. The first question factored into the Motivation to Comply Scale asks “How much attention do you pay to your health?” .A significant difference existed between males (88%) and females (96%) reporting “a lot/some attention to health”. In the Fisher sample where the breakdown of gender is mere equally distributed than Nazareth, the females still report more attention to health (94%), than their male counterparts (87%). It is also interesting to note that the higher the self-reported grade point average, the higher the amount of attention paid to health issues. Ninety-seven percent of students with over a 3.5, paid a great deal of attention to health, compared to 85% with a 2.5 grade point average or below. In the overall sample, age seems to influence attention to health issues, attention paid to wellness programs, and concern for personal safety. In both sample populations, students who are 18 or younger (46%) pay “a lot of attention? to personal safety. In this age category (18 or less), Fisher students reported(81%) at the “a lot of 57 attention” level, compared to Nazareth (56%) . The other two elements of Motivation to Comply are awareness of campus safety services and use of these services. Awareness and usage of campus safety services seemed to be higher among Nazareth students. Specifically, 99% of the Nazareth population reported being aware that of campus safety services being available to them, compared to 94% of Fisher students. Those who take advantage of the services are more likely to be from Nazareth, and more likely to be female (43% as compared to 21% for male) and older students. E . l 5 '1'1' 3 2 . There was no significant difference between the Nazareth and Fisher samples on the perceived susceptibility scale (see Table 4.3). Within the combined sample however, females were more likely to score in the high range than males (33% vs 18%). Of those with a grade point average of 2.5 or less, 43% scored low on perceived susceptibility compared with only 28% of those with those with a grade point average of 3.5 or better. Grade point average continues to be a factor within the scores of the safety belief scales. Perceived likelihood of being a victim of property crime is also an item included in the Perceived Susceptibility Scale. Freshman (65%) were most likely to select “highly unlikely to be a victim” compared to all 58 other upperclass students in the combined sample, with no other classes reporting over 59%. In speaking to the issue of invulnerability, there was no significant difference between students at the two colleges with regard to the question of their perceived likelihood of becoming a victim of property crime. Eighty- two percent of the total sample report that their chances are less/much less than others, with no significant differences with respect to gender, age, race/ethnicity, ’semesters on campus, and grade point average. When asked about perceived likelihood of becoming a victim of violent personal crime, the percentages are very similar with 83% of the total sample stating that they are unlikely/very unlikely to become a victim» In the combined sample, somewhat more females (17%) believe they that are very likely to become victims of a violent personal crime than their male peers (13%). When reporting their own feelings of safety while on campus, nearly all Nazareth and Fisher students report they feel safe/very safe during daylight hours, 100% and 99% respectively. Not surprisingly fewer report feeling safe on campus after dark. Eighty-nine percent of Nazareth students report feeling safe/very safe in comparison to 83% Fisher students reporting the same way to this question. Males (95%) feel much safer than women, with 82% saying they feel safe/very safe on campus after dark. 59 In terms of feeling safe in their dorms, both populations reported high levels of perceived safety. Ninety-six percent of Fisher students report feeling safe/very safe in the dorms compared to 99% of Nazareth students. When looking at grade point averages, 100% of students who have a 2.5 or less report feeling safe/very safe in the dorms. This reinforces the relationship between low grade point average and high feelings of perception of safety in the residence halls. When asked whether or not they believe that there are specific precautions a person can take which will reduce his or her chances of being a victim of a violent personal crime, both Nazareth and Fisher students overwhelming agreed with this statement (96%). Juniors at Nazareth agreed with this statement mere often than any other class. B . l S 'l E 2 . M' . . I' This scale contains items for subjective assessment of the effect of crime victimization on college, work, and personal life. When asked how much do they worry about being a victim.on campus, 45% of the Nazareth students reported “a great deal/some” in comparison to Fisher's 37% response. Female students at both institutions worry much more than their male peers, (52% vs. 16%). This is a significant difference at the p<.05 and one of the largest gender differences in the study. Students with higher grade point averages worry more about being a victim as 60 demonstrated by 88% with a 3.0 or higher responding “a great deal/some" versus 43% with a 2.5 or less responding “little or no worry”. Forty-six percent of seniors and freshmen in both samples worry at about the same level, somewhat higher than their sophomore and junior classmates (36%). When asked to what extent concern for safety on campus interferes with doing things they like to do, most of the total sample replied “little/none”, with an 85% response to this question. A small significant difference exists between men (10%) and women (17%) responding “.a great deal/some” extent to the which concern for safety interferes with doing what they would like. E . l E E' E I 1' E . Perceived benefits are beliefs that compliance with recommended safety precautions will reduce the threat of crime victimization. An underlying theme to this scale is the feeling of personal control over individual safety and general ability to reduce victimization. Students were asked the power the college had to reduce their chance of becoming violent crime victims. There was no significant difference between Nazareth and Fisher in their responses. Seventy-five percent of females in the combined sample responded that the college has “a great deal” or “some” power, compared to the males (40%) in the combined sample who believed that the college had “little” or “none” to do 61 with reducing the chance of victimization. Freshmen students (78%) were also more likely to believe the college has a great deal of power in this area (78% vs 68% of upperclassmen). When asked about their personal power to reduce the chance of victimization, 85% responded that they had "a great deal” or “some” power, with females responding a bit higher (87%) in comparison to males (81%). On a similar theme, over 97% of the entire sample “agreed” or “agreed very tmxflf that there are specific precautions a person can take which will reduce his or her chances of being a victim. A compiled list of the suggested safety precautions from each college are listed in Tables 4.4 and 4.5. Respondents had an opportunity within the survey to list safety precautions students could take on their campus. Students at both institutions listed “Don't walk alone/Don't walk alone at night7*with the highest frequency. In the area of assessment of their ability to physically defend themselves from attempted crime, the gender variable shows a significant difference at the .05 level, with 86% of the males in the combined sample reporting that they were “likely” to be able to defend themselves in comparison to their female peers, only 57% of whom see themselves as “likely” to defend themselves. 62 FM 4.4 SAFETY PRECAUTIONS RECORD- BY FISHER STUDENTS Precaution Don’t walk alone/don’t walk alone at night. 205 Be alert/aware of surroundings 49 Take self-defense class 48 Cuwpeppmpny/m 35 Lock your door 19 Avoid poorly lit streets/paths 17 Stay away fiom/don’t leave parties with Hangers 15 Use an escort service 14 Be aware of and use campus safety services 12 Use common sense 12 Carry personal beeper/alarms 1 1 Tell people where you’re going 11 Sayincontrol atparties/don’tdrinktoonmch 9 Take precautions 7 Walk with confidence 5 Don’t hesitate to fight back 6 Don’t make anyone mad 4 Use Blue phones 4 Don’t respond to fights 3 Don’t can-y lots of money 3 Haveyom'keysreadybeforereachingdoor/car 2 Carry a whistle 2 Don’t dress provocatively 2 Don’t prop doors 1 Don’t provoke trouble/dress provocatively 1 Wear light clothing 1 n=307 63 Figure 4.5 SAFETY PRECAUTIONS RECORD- BY NAZARETH STUDENTS Preeaution Don’t walk alone/don’t walk alone at night 286 Take self-defame class 120 Carry Security Escort 102 Be alert/aware of surroundings Lockyourdoor Avoid poorly lit meets/paths Stay in control at parties/don’t drink too much Stay away fiom/don’t leave parties with strangers Beawareofandusecampussafetyservices Use common sense Canypepper spray/mace Use an escort service Walk with confidence Tell pe0ple where you’re going Don’thesitatetofightback Haveyomkeysreadybeforereachingdoor/car Don’t respond to fights Don’t make anyone mad Carryawhistle Don’tdressprovocatively Don’t can-y lots of money Don’t provoke trouble/dress provocatively Don’tcommitacrime 54 E . l E . I l' E . Perceived barriers are subjective reasons why taking suggested safety precautions are not met with a positive response. The questions included in this scale assess students' negative perceptions of the recommended safety behaviors, including peers' opinions of safety precautions and convenience of use. Nazareth students appear to be less bothered by taking safety precautions than the Fisher sample. When asked if they agreed with the statement.TSometimes I'm.embarrassed to insist on certain safety precautions when I'm out with friends", more Nazareth students (78%) disagreed with the statement than Fisher (68%). When asked about the amount of time required to take safety precautions, 43% of the Fisher students report that they don't take precautions because the behaviors take too much time, in comparison to 35% of Nazareth students. Finally in this category, students were then asked to rate the degree of hassle for a series of safety precautions. The list of safety precautions are rated by Nazareth and Fisher samples in Table 4.6. The safety precautions which are statistically significant at the .05 level are asterisked. Figure 4.6 65 RATINGS OF SAFETY PRECAUTIONS - PERCENTS ‘p < .05. Qautioa:lngeneral,hownmdtofalnsdearethefoflowingufetyprecmfions? PRECAUTION Nuareth% Fisher-96 Asldngafiieultowalkoncampuswithyou VeryEasy 41 37 afisrdark Easy 42 4O Allassle 8" 12‘ Don’tDo 9 ll Callingafiiendtowalkoncampuswithyou Veryksy 28 30 aflerchrk Easy 42 36 AI-hsale 18 19 Don’tDo 12 15 Callingthecarqmspoliceaerviceforeacort VeryBasy 20 18 mmafiertheslmttlelnsstoppedfir Easy 29 32 thenight AI-lassle 21 23 Don’tDo 31 28 Avoiding" poorlylitpathsoncanpus. VeryEasy 26 24 Easy 50 49 AHassle 16 18 Don’tDo 8 10 lodung' dormroomatmght‘ VetyEasy 82 77 Easy 14‘ 19‘ Al-lasale l l Don’tDo 3 3 Keepmg' dammcmwmdows' locked VeryEasy 39‘ 32' Easy 23 22 Al'hsale 9 12 Don’tDo 28" 35‘ Clash oath restdace' lllldoorsyoufind VeryEasy 39‘ 31‘ pruppedopm. Easy 37 38 Al-lassle 7 8 Don’tDo 17‘ 24‘ Arranginforfirst—timedatestooccurin VeryEasy 31 30 fimiliaraunmmdinga. Easy 48 45 AHassle 5' 9‘ Don’tDo l6 l6 Avoilingleavingapartyalonewithanew VeryEasy 33 29 W. Easy 47 43 Al-lassle 6' 11" Don’tDo 14 17 Noufytng' '- secunty' ofausptctous" personson VeryEasy 24 20 campmorintherssidmce' hall. Easy 48‘ 40‘ Al-laaale 15‘ 22‘ Don’tDo 29 18 66 S E E l . I | . The Safety Belief Model postulates that several safety belief variables, combined with a student's demographic characteristics and certain life experiences, could predict the likelihood of following certain simple and common safety precautions. Such precautions include calling a friend to walk with after dark, using an escort service, avoiding poorly lit paths, locking doors and windows, securing open doors, notifying security personnel of suspicious persons, and arranging for first-time dates to occur in familiar surroundings (Lenski et a1, 1996). The Safety Behavior Intention scale was computed from the respondents' ratings of the likelihood they would take the same safety precautions listed in the Perceived Barriers scale. The responses were summed to a safety behavior intent score. Responses to the intention scale questions are provided in Table 4.7. For each of the safety precautions (with the exception of “Calling campus security for an escort”) , females in both samples reported a much greater likelihood of taking specific precautions. Females are more likely to call/ask a friend to walk with them, avoid poorly lit paths, lock their dorm door at night, avoid leaving a party with new acquaintance, arrange for date to occur in familiar surroundings and would notify security if they saw a suspicious person on campus. Table 4.7 67 SAFETY BEHAVIOR INTENTION - LIKELIHOOD OF TAKING SPECIFIC SAFETY PRECAUTIONS DURING THE MONTH, INDEPENDENT Z-TESTS FOR PERCENTAGES v Preeaution Nazareth Fisher at 1' m . f l. Call/askafiiendtowalkwithyourafierdark Definitely will 3%" 15%" 3%" 24%" Probably will 11%" 41%" 19%" 40%" Probably won’t 44% 38% 39% 31% Definitely won’t 42% 6%" 39%" 5%" 2. Call campus security for an escort after dark Definitely will 1% 1% 2% 5% Probably will 3%" . 9%" 9%" 16%" Probably won’t 35%" 64%" 36%" 56%" Definitely won’t 61%" 26%" 53%" 23%" 3. Avoid poorly lit paths after dark Definitely will 6%" 33%" 7%" 38%" Probably will 28%" 49%" 31%" 42%" Probably won’t 41%" 15%" 39%" 17%" Definitely won’t 25%" 3%" 23%" 3%" 4. Lock dorm room at night Definitelywill sow lass/a- 70%* 377» Probably will 25%" 8%" 19%" 10%" Probably won’t 18%" 3%" 10%" 2%" Definitely won’t 7%" 1%" 1% 1% 5. Keep dorm windows locked at night Definitely will 13%" 24%" 19%" 27%" Probably will 27% 25% 21% 23% Probably won’t 39% 38% 31% 30% Definitely won’t 21% 13%" 29%" 20%" 6. Close a propped door in your dorm Definitely will 14%‘I 26%" 17% 24% Probably will 25%" 42%" 35% 40% Probably won’t 48%" 29%" 35% 31% Definitely won’t 13%" 3%" 13%" 5%" 68 Table 4.7 SAFETY BEHAVIOR INTENTION - LIKELIHOOD OF TAKING SPECIFIC SAFETY PRECAUTIONS DURING THE MONTH. INDEPENDENT Z-TESTS FOR PERCENTAGES 7tAnumefindueuuxnminfimflhrnmmwmmms Definitely will 12%" 42%" 1 1%" 43%" Probably will 33%" 48%'I 39% 43% Probably won’t 38%" 7%" 35%" 9%" Definitely won’t 17%" 3%" 15%" 5%" 8. Avoid leaving party alone with a new acquaintance Definitely will 11%" 50%" 9%" 53%" Probably will 26%" 42%" 38% 36% Probably won’t 43%" 7%" 36%" 8%" Definitely won’t 20%" 1%" 17%" 3%" 91kflfinnannyflyuuneasmmkkmspuummmcmmpu Ikfimuhnfin 11%" 29%" 1&%" 21%" Probably will 39% 45% 45% 43% Probably won’t 34%" 34% 34% 27% Definitely won’t 10%" 5% 5% 3% "p<05 Not surprisingly, when the Safety Behavior Scores were summed, the females scored significantly higher at both institutions. However, the difference between the genders at the two schools was similar,- men in the high score range (7% Nazareth and 10% Fisher), range(44% Nazareth and 46% Fisher). females in the high score Table 4.8 shows the distribution of Safety Behavior Intention scores by gender for each of the two colleges. 69 Table 4.8 SAFETY BEHAVIOR INTENTION Z-‘I'ES‘I‘ FOR PERCENTAGES COMPARISON ‘GROWWSNGENDERJICUKJIEEI Safety Behavior Intention Score Nazareth Fisher at 1' III 1' High Score 7%" 44%" 10%" 46%" Mid-range Score 20%" 37%" 25% 32% Low Score 73%" 19%" 65%" 22%" " p < .05. One of the study's specific purposes was to address the question of whether the Safety Belief Model is useful in predicting students' behavioral intentions. Research questions were developed to examine if a relationship exists between any of the safety belief variables, modifying variables (includes demographic variables, prior victimization and college) and the safety behavioral intentions, as summarized by the Safety Behavior Intention Scale. From the research questions, null hypotheses were developed and tested in the data analysis. The first three hypotheses dealt with the dependent outcome, Safety Behavior Intention: 1). There will be no significant relationship between any of the safety belief variables and safety behavior intention. 2). There will be no significant relationship between any of the demographic variables and safety behavior intention. 3). There will be no significant relationship between any of the enabling variables and safety behavior intention. 70 In order to test these three null hypotheses and the relationship between the safety belief scales, demographic variables, enabling variables and safety behavior intention, a multiple regression was performed. Safety behavior intention was the dependent variable. The statistical package used to analyze the data checked for multicollinearity prior to including the variable in the regression. The analysis yielded nine variables which accounted for 64% of the variance in the dependent variable, Safety Behavior Intention. The most significant predictor of safety behavior intention was Perceived Barriers, which by itself accounted for approximately 40% of the total variance (Beta=.719). The next most important predictors included Readiness to Act(Beta=.294), Gender(Beta=.288),and Motivation to Comply(Beta=.l45). Other factors which had a smaller influence on safety behavior intention included semesters on campus, college, enabling factors, age and perceived benefits variables, although, these variables accounted for a very small proportion of the variance. The stepwise regression is summarized in Table 4.9. The regression indicated that safety behavior intention does vary significantly with safety beliefs (perceived barriers, readiness to act, motivation to comply and perceived benefits), demographic variables (gender, age, and semesters on campus), and enabling variables. Therefore, the first three null hypotheses are rejected. 71 Table 4.9 STEPWISE MULTIPLE REGRESSION ON SAFETY BEHAVIOR INTENTION WITH DENKXHUUHHCS Variable Beta R R, Increase in R, 1. Perceived Barriers Scale .719 .634 .402 - 2. Readiness to Act .294 .777 .604 .202 3. Gender .288 .734 .539 .065 4. Motivation to Comply .145 .782 .612 .073 5. Semesters on Campus .118 .794 .631 .019 6.Coflege .076 .787 .619 .012 7. Enabling Factors .074 .791 .626 .007 8. Age .072 .796 .634 .008 9. Perceived Benefits .056 .798 .636 .002 W The revised Safety Belief Model (Safety Belief Model II) postulates that several belief variables, combined with demographic and certain life experiences, could predict the likelihood of using a personal safety beeper. Independent 2- Tests for percentages and a multiple regression analysis were used to analyze the data on beeper usage and relationship with demographic, enabling and safety belief variables. Tables 4.10 and 4.11 provide information on who possesses a beeper on the Nazareth Campus and how often they use it. The most common characteristics of beeper owners are: they are mostly women, who are freshman, 18 years old or less, and self-report their grade point average over 2.5. Females(72%)report a significant and dramatic comparison to 72 males(19%) in possession of this safety intervention. As students' ages increase, as well as the length of time reported on campus, the rate of possession steadily decreases. And finally, as grade point average increases, so does the percentage of beeper owners (see Table 4.10) . Table 4.10 SAFETY DEVICE USERS CA‘I'EGORIZED BY DEMOGRAPHIC VARIABLES Z-TEST FOR PERCENTAGES (BASE: NAZARETH COLLEGE STUDENTS) Variable Possess Beeper YES NO Gender male 19% 81%" female 72%" 28% Class Standing Freshman 81%" 19% Sophomore 66%" 34%" Junior 38% 62%" Senior 43% 57%" Age 18 or less 82%" 18% 19 65%" 35%" 20 42% 58%" 21 + 40% 60%" Race White 62% 38% Non-White ( 62% 38% Semesters on Campus f < 1 80%" 20% 1 - 2 50% 50%" 5 + 38% 62%" Grade Point Average Under 2.5 35% 65%" 2.5 - 3.0 59%" 41% 3.0 - 3.5 64%" 36% 3.5 - 4.0 63%" 37% "p<.05 73 Among Nazareth students, 59% report some use of the beeper, forty-one percent saying they use it “all the time”, 12% most of the time and only 3% of those who possess a beeper, fail to use it at all (see Table 4.11). TMSUWAMI RY or BEEPER USAGE (BASE: NAZARETH COLLEGE STUDENTS) Use beeper all the time 41% Use beeper most of the time 12% Use beeper not very ofien 6% Have beeper but never use it 3% DomMpouus 3@% n=SZ3 ' The remainder of the data analysis examines the relationship between safety device usage and safety beliefs, enabling variables, demographic variables and safety behavior intention. Specific hypotheses will be addressed with z-Tests for percentages and a multiple regression analysis. W: Safety device users will report higher scores on all safety belief variables. Table 4.12 presents the findings of a Z-Test for percentages with significant differences asterisked at the p<.05 level. In all the safety belief variables, beeper users scored higher than non-users with the exception of the 74 Perceived Barriers Scale and the Readiness to Act Scale. Therefore, the hypothesis was rejected at the .05 level. Table 4.12 SAFETY BELIEF VARIABLES BY BEEPER USAGE, Z-TEST FOR PERCENTAGES Ehkqanks Uuflkan YES NO Motivati' 'on to Comply High 43%" 17%" Mid- e 43% 39% Low 14%" 44%" Pauhafihmqmfimy Hum 30% 2S% hfldkuge 4T%" 3T% LOW 23%" 41%" ‘HmmwflSwmny High 41%" 20%" hfldkmme 2E6 19% Low 34%" 61%" Paahuflkmfim High 32%" 26%" rhfi¢mmmz . 4H6 4M% Low ‘ 2T% 30% PuummdBufias High 21w 39w Mid-Range 35% 35% Low 44%" 26%" RedhwmoAm High 28% 34% Mid-Range 409/0" 270/“. Low 32% 39% "p<.05 75 H¥DQLhEBiS_5: There will be no significant differences for safety device users when categorized by demographic variables. Table 4.10 categorizes safety device users by demographic variables. Significant differences exist between beeper users and non-users on gender, class standing, age, race, semesters on campus and grade point average. Hypotheses 5 is rejected at the p.<05. level. Wis—6.: Safety device users will report higher scores on the enabling variables. Table 4.13 presents the findings of a z-Test for percentages with enabling factors by beeper usage. Beeper users only scored higher in the mad-range level, not the high level. It doesn't appear that prior life experience impacts the use of beepers in this study. Overall, beeper users do report somewhat higher scores, therefore Hypothesis 6 is accepted. Table 4.13 ENABLING FACTORS BY BEEPER USAGE Z-‘I'EST FOR PERCENTAGES Enabling Factor Scale Use Beeper YES NO) 18%. 1F% :3: «e 53%" 4W%" low zen 4r% "p<05 76 W: Safety device users will report higher scores on safety behavior intention. Table 4.14 presents the findings of a Z-Test for percentage between the two dependent variables, safety behavior intention and beeper usage. At the p.<.05 level, there was a significant difference between beeper users and non-users at all three levels. A relationship appears to exist between the safety belief intention and one's use of a safety device. The higher one scores on safety behavior intention, the greater their likelihood of using a beeper. Based on this finding, Hypothesis 7 is accepted. Table 4.14 SAFETY BEHAVIOR INTENTION BY BEEPER USAGE Z-TEST FOR PERCENTAGES Safety Behavior Intention Use Beeper YES .NO Hmm 47%" 20%" Mid-Range 36% 30% low 17%" 50%" "p<05 ~ In addition to the z-Tests, a multiple regression analysis was performed to determine which demographic, enabling and safety belief variables are significant predictors of beeper usage. The analysis yielded three variables which accounted for 33% of the variance in the dependent variable, Beeper Usage. The most significant predictor of beeper usage was gender(Beta=.325), which by 77 itself accounted for approximately 20% of the total variance. The other two predictor variables, Motivation to Comply(Beta=.278) and Semesters on Campus(Beta=.274), accounted for the remainder of the variance. The stepwise regression is summarized in Table 4.15. Table 4.15 STEPWISE MULTIPLE REGRESSION ON BEEPER USAGE WITH DEMOGRAPHICS \hmhbk Ban R. R, InueuwbnR, 1. Gender .325 .446 .199 - 2. Motivation to Comply .278 .577 .334 .135 3. Semesters on Campus .274 .517 .268 .066 In an article in a recent trade journal for college housing officers, a vendor of electronic alarm systems stated that alarm and beeper systems can enhance an institution's overall security program by offering constant protection. This particular system comes complete with a beeper for all users (similar to the one in use at Nazareth). The vendor claims that. “electronic/beeper systems are particularly well-suited for schools since they will not fall asleep on the job, cannot be led into temptation, and can sound at the first sign of trouble” (Probst, 1996, p. 22). Although not a specific aspect of this study on safety behavior intention and beeper usage, it seemed significant 78 to include campus crime statistics for Nazareth College and St. John Fisher College. Table 4.16 includes crime statistics for the period that the survey was distributed and collected (Fall, 1996). Crime on campus has held fairly steady over the past five years, although there has been an increase in grand and petty larceny and motor vehicle theft on both campuses. Also included in Table 4.16 is the notation of the year that Nazareth acquired the Security Eacort System and made beepers available to its community. 1993 and 1994 showed a decline in crime on the Nazareth campus, but in the past two years the statistics are comparable with St. John Fisher. Table 4.16 ' CAMPUS CRIME STATISTICS 1992 - 1996 BY COLLEGE Crime Categories 1992 1993 1994 1995 1996 NC SJFC NC SJFC NC SJFC NC SJFC NC SJFC hhmfln&flkm~ 0 0 0 0 (1 0 0 0 0 0 Negligent Homicide Forcible Rape 0 4 0 1 0 0 1 0 0 0 Non-forcible Sex 0 0 0 0 0 1 0 0 1 0 (flfinus Rummy 0 0 0 0 (1 0 1 0 0 0 lMflMWflwdAuth 0 7 (3 1 1 3 0 3 0 3 Burglary 6 7 1 2 1 l8 4 10 3 1 l Larceny (Grand & 80 101 66 72 34 85 91 73 114 9?. Petty) Motor Vehicle 0 0 0 0 0 1 0 2 5 4 ram Anon 1 0 0 0 0 2 0 1 0 0 NC: Nazareth College. SJFC: St. John Fisher College Nazareth installed the Security Escort System in Fall, 1993 79 Summary: Seven primary research questions were addressed in this study. The focus of the first three was on the relationship between safety belief variables, enabling variables, and demographic variables on safety behavior intention. The final four questions focused on the relationship between safety belief variables, enabling variables, demographic variables, safety behavior intention on beeper usage. The specific focus of the study was two-fold. One was to examine the predictive value of the Safety Belief Model and the second was to examine the relationship between use of personal safety devices and safety behavioral intentions. Statistical analysis included z—Tests for percentages and multiple regressions. The seven hypotheses were formulated at the .05 level of significance to test relationships between segments on a variety of variables. The results are summarized below: Hypothesia_1: There will be no significant differences between any of the safety belief variables and safety behavior intention. The null hypothesis was rejected at the .05 level. Hypothesis_2; There will be no significant differences between any of the demographic variables and safety behavior intention. The null hypothesis was rejected at the .05 level. Hypothesis_1: There will be no significant differences between any of the enabling variables and safety behavior intention. The null hypothesis was rejected at the .05 level. Safety device users will report higher scores on all of the safety belief variables. The hypothesis was rejected at the .05 level. 8O Hypothesis_5: There will be no significant differences for safety device users when categorized by demographic variables. The null hypothesis was rejected at the .05 level. Hypothesis_fi: Safety device users will report higher scores on the enabling variables. Hypothesis 6 was accepted at the .05 level. Hypothesis_1: Safety device users will report higher scores on safety behavior intention. Hypothesis 7 was accepted at the .05 level. Chapter 5 SUMMARY, FINDINGS, CONCLUSIONS AND RECOMMENDATIONS W The purpose of this study was two-fold. A prediction study design was used to describe the relationship between safety related beliefs, demographic and enabling variables and the intention to use precautionary strategies. A model designed by Lenski in 1992, Safety Belief Model I, was modified to include an additional dependent variable. This additional variable was the likelihood of using a personal safety device. The study examined the relationship between safety behavioral intentions and the use of personal safety devices. All residential students at Nazareth College and St. John Fisher College, two small, private colleges located in Rochester, New York were sent a copy of The Campus Safety Survey (a seven page survey instrument), along with a cover letter, a return postcard and an addressed return envelope. This material was sent to the residential students' campus mailboxes during the fourth week of the Fall Semester, 1996. After the initial mailing, all residential students were sent a reminder postcard and information of how to obtain a second questionnaire if their first one was misplaced or 81 82 lost. A total of 1528 surveys were distributed and 1003 were returned. Of those returned, 997 questionnaires were usable, representing a 65% return rate. Research questions which focused on the relationships between safety belief variables, modifying variables, safety behavior intention and safety device users were formulated into seven testable hypotheses. These hypotheses were tested at the .05 level of significance. Descriptive statistics were used to report characteristics of the respondents. Differences between segments were evaluated for significance using z-tests for percentages, and analysis of variance for means. Multiple regression analysis was used for each of the dependent variables to identify those factors which have the greatest impact on predicting safety behavior intention and the use of a personal safety device. Conclusions The major conclusion of this study is that it is possible to predict intended safety device behavior and safety device usage by using a combination of safety belief, enabling and demographic variables. The Safety Belief Model II was supported by the study results. Specific variables and scales which contributed to the prediction of safety belief intention and beeper usage are discussed in this section. 83 SafetLBehaxioLIntr-mtion The first three hypotheses focused on the safety belief variables, demographic variables, enabling variables and safety intention behavior. The factors which accounted for the most variance in the dependent variable, safety behavior intention, were Perceived Barriers, Readiness to Act, Gender and Motivation to Comply. Differences between the college samples were significant in a number of the safety belief scales. Within the Perceived Barriers Scale, Nazareth students appear to be less bothered by taking safety precautions. Fisher students scored at a higher rate than Nazareth, responding that most safety precautions were a hassle, took too much time, and that they would not likely call security or a friend for assistance. In comparison to Fisher, Nazareth students listed a much greater number of safety precautions that a students can take to reduce their likelihood of being a victim. Nazareth reported a higher attendance rate at safety programs and within Motivation to Comply, Nazareth scored significantly higher than Fisher in the top two ranges, demonstrating a higher level of motivation in practicing safe behavior. Overall, students' attention to health, wellness and personal safety, along with awareness of campus safety services, contributed to prediction of intended precautionary behavior. Under the Perceived Severity Scale, Nazareth students 84 reported that they worry about crime victimization much more than Fisher students and that they are more likely to use campus safety services (i.e. such as using campus safety personnel, calling a friend to walk with them after dark, using blue phones or reporting suspicious persons to campus safety). There were no significant differences between Nazareth and Fisher on perceived susceptibility scale. Both sample populations saw themselves as relatively safe and less likely than their peers to be a victim of crime. In the combined sample, freshman saw themselves as highly unlikely to be a victim.more so than the reporting upperclass students. Although both campus samples feel very safe during the day, Fisher students reported feeling less safe at night. In the overall comparison of the Fisher and Nazareth samples and safety behavior intention, significant differences existed between the genders within both colleges. Within the score ranges, females reported considerable higher safety behavior intention scores which is consistent with other research, including Lenski's study in 1992. Saien¥_DB¥ine;nfia9£ A claim in a marketing brochure for a personal safety device states that the device system delivers round-the— clock protection indoors, outdoors, in elevators, even underground in tunnels and basements (Security Escort, 85 1994). Another marketing piece claims that their electronic device system enhances an institution's overall security by offering constant protection (Probst, 1996). This study provides information on student usage of a beeper system and the relationship of this intervention with other safety belief values and demographic information of the sample. During the Fall Semester, 1996, 886 personal safety devices were in use at Nazareth College. Of the subscribers, 473 were residential students, 290 were utilized by commuters and the remaining beepers were used by staff, faculty members and security personnel. The most startling distribution is the gender breakdown. Of the 886 total, 810 beepers are used by women compared to 56 used by the male population (Struble, 1997). In the reporting sample, beeper users were mainly women, freshmen, 18 years old or less, have lived on campus one semester or less, and report their grade point average at 2.5 or over. Those that possess a beeper, tend to use it. Most people who have a beeper report using it all the time. The remaining four hypotheses relate to beeper usage and safety belief, demographic, and enabling variables. Beeper users scored much higher on the following safety belief scales: motivation to comply, perceived susceptibility, perceived severity and perceived benefits. Enabling variables didn't have much influence on the difference between beeper users and non-users, similar to 86 their lack of impact on safety behavior intention. One of the most significant findings in the study was the relationship between safety device usage and safety behavior intention. The higher a student scored in the safety behavior intention, the greater their likelihood of using a beeper. This finding demonstrated that the Safety Belief Model II was successful in predicting the likelihood of personal safety device usage, yet some of the key drivers for behavioral intention were different from those for beeper usage. Significant predictors of beeper usage included gender, motivation to comply and semesters on campus. Significant predictors of safety behavior intention included perceived barriers, readiness to act, gender and motivation to comply. When Nazareth students were asked their perception of the effectiveness of the beeper system.an preventing crime, 81% reported that it is effective compared to those (19%) who believe it is not. When asked about the impact the beeper has on feelings of personal safety, 72% of the Nazareth sample reports that it makes them feel safer. What is troubling about the usage of the beepers is the amount of freshmen and those on campus for their first semester that choose to use the beeper. There is a noticeable decline in use as students age and the longer they live in a residential environment. As students become more familiar with the environment and more comfortable with their community, does their attention to personal safety 87 issues decline? What is unknown about the adoption of the personal safety device is whether the initial decision to use a beeper is solely the students. Parental influence can be very strong for a large number of college freshmen. There may be great pressure or strong encouragement from freshmen parents for these students, particularly female, to sign up for the system. I J' I' E E . When a college campus is perceived by students to be unsafe, those feelings represent a major obstacle in the achievement of the educational goals of the student and the institution. It is significant for educational leaders to understand that any incidents that interfere with the emotional, psychological or physical well-being of students represent an obstacle of the achievement of the educational mission of the institution. Unfortunately, college responses to crime have historically been reactive, rather that proactive. You can survey students five years in a row and learn that they desire additional lighting, but what motivates action in terms of adding lighting, increasing security patrol, trimming landscape issues are typically either an increase of incidents of crime or a significantly violent incident on the campus. College administrators need to pay attention to the environmental issues on a regular basis, without waiting 88 for legislation to dictate it or for an incident to require a response. The results of this study suggest that college admdnistrators need to take a look at the bigger picture of student behavior and victimization on campus. The literature on human behavior, risk behavior and health issues speaks to the issue of personal safety and should be examined further. The perceived barriers factor was a major consideration and predictor of safety behavior intention. Students who perceive precautions to be a hassle or an inconvenience are less likely to take the extra effort to ensure their safety or the safety of their community members. Those administrators responsible for setting up systems for building entrance/exit, for travel across campus (especially at night), and for ways to activate campus safety personnel need to solicit feedback from students on ways to make precautions easier to use and more student accessible. Students were able to list precautions rather easily as shown in Tables 4.4 and 4.5 in Chapter Four. .As suggested by Lenski in her findings (1992), students that describe themselves as more concerned or attentive to health, wellness or personal safety were more likely to plan on taking better precautions than students who were less concerned with health issues. Educators from other areas of responsibility can be utilized in a collaborative way to reach all students on the importance of taking care of one's 89 self while on campus and off. The findings of this study provide information about populations who believe that they are safer than others or that threat of victimization is not a large concern for them. Specifically, males, students with lower grade point averages, students who have a long history in residence halls could be targeted for educational programming on personal safety issues. All students need to better understand the risks prevalent on college campuses and that there are measures they can take to reduce the risk of victimization. While not present in this study, but worth mentioning as perception of victimization is discussed, is the role of the College and the perception of the collegiate environment depicted in college brochures and handbooks. If the environments are described as safe, welcoming and family- like, do applicants and later, entering students get an unrealistic assessment of the college campus? Student perceptions begin with print material and information from college representatives, often before they step foot on the campus. This is a good time to begin to discuss the individual's responsibility for safety and for self-care. The findings demonstrated that safety workshops are important and Nazareth students in particular, felt that is was a precaution that students should engage in. Educational workshops on personal safety, self-defense classes and any other opportunities to reinforce use of 90 existing campus safety interventions should be offered throughout the college students collegiate experience. Covering this topic once a year at Orientation sessions clearly is not sufficient, as demonstrated by our upperclass data. Safety interventions such as the personal safety device should be investigated in depth. Clearly, the beeper is seen as effective by a large part of the population at Nazareth. But does it really make students safer? As seen by the Campus Crime Statistics in Table 4.16, the crime rate did not decrease significantly. Many of the students who possess a beeper report high usage of it, but does it provide a false sense of security? While using the beeper, do students feel more comfortable engaging in risky behavior? Printed material on such interventions need to be responsible and accurate on what the intervention does provide so that users can be knowledgeable about the product and its limitations. 1' . . The results of this study are directly applicable to small, private colleges. In addition, the populations at both schools in the study are ethnically homogeneous and the overwhelming majority of white respondents limit the ability to draw conclusions about safety precautions for campus populations that are more diverse. Another limdtation was that the study was completed in 91 the first five weeks of the semester. It was done at this time to have students have a level of comfort with their environment and establish patterns of daily behaviors. If the study been done at a different point of the year, it may have resulted in differences in reporting of safety behaviors. The general feelings of comfort with an environment may have decreased the use of precautionary behavior as demonstrated by less attention being displayed by upperclass students and by those that have a history of residing in the halls. Jessor's work on risk behavior and problem behavior theory is limited in its breadth. His work has been done at the secondary level and has not been used with college-aged students. More research should be done with his theory with college populations. A significant limitation of this study is the definition of safety. Research on campus crime has dealt with a very limited view of crime on campus, with a great deal of emphasis on property or violent crime. College admdnistrators are cognizant of the growing cases of hate speech, harassment and even the issues of victimization occurring through cyberspace and through technological mediums. During the Spring Semester, 1997 an incident of hate speech occurred on Nazareth's campus. Racial slurs were written on a resident student's dorm room door. The campus community responded with anger and frustration and this was 92 illustrated by student responses in a local newspaper. A student victim stated that although he had a beeper to alert security if he is attacked, he does not feel safe as a result of this incident. Students felt that the administration was not doing enough and that students' safety was not guaranteed (Wentzel, 1997). It is irresponsible and dated to define safety only in property or physical terms. Another limitation of the study was that it used behavioral intention rather than actual self-reported behavior. It is prospective rather that retrospective, since the focus was on factors which influence behavioral intention. It is possible that the safety behavior intention reported by the respondents would not coincide with their final actions. There is no prediction of the frequency of actual safety related behavior from the samples. WW .As suggested by Lenski in her dissertation, there are several possibilities for future research. Because of the overlap of items for different scales, the predictive power of the scales was difficult to differentiate. To clarify components of the Safety Belief Model, the items should be reviewed in depth to learn which questions accounted for the most power to predict. If the work which has been completed in this study was to be examined in conjunction with 93 Lenski's, Safety Belief Model I could be more specific and be clarified. Overall, this type of research has been performed only at a large, private institution and now at two, small private colleges. All the populations have been characterized as homogeneous and with large residential components. Because campus crime impacts commuter populations, as well as campuses which have a greater diversity in ethnicity and age, the Campus Safety Survey could be utilized as an assessment tool on a number of other campuses to learn more about behavioral intentions of college students nationally. This study focused on one intervention, a personal safety device. There are a variety of campus safety interventions in place on college campuses, and they should be evaluated in a complex way. It should not be left up to the manufacturer of these products to demonstrate their effectiveness on college campuses. Educators and administrators need to take responsibility for providing the best safety measures for the students who are apart of their community. A research basis can take some of the guess work and ambiguity out of planning programs to highlight and encourage students to take care of themselves while they are on college campuses. APPENDIX A APPENDIX A THE CADIPU S SAFETY SURVEY Nazareth College Fall, 1996 Mommasmwybyphdmm'rbymmsndmndumfimnnflmwmvflm provided. Itshouldtalnsnomoreduulsm. Allrupcnsuwillbahustrictlycaifiduaislandmindividmlwillbe idmtifiedin-Iywaymthesmdymort. Myoul 1. Somepeoplearequitecmcemedabomhealth.whileothusarenot. Hownaichsttemiondoyoupsyto yourhealth? IDantofsttution 20mm 3Dlidesttution 4Clnoattention 2. Howmchsttatiudoyoupsytowebessprogmnsofinducamus? IDalotofattuItirm 20mm 30mm 4Dnosnention 3.1agenenlhowmchsttenfiondoyoupsytoymnpersonalsafetymcanpus? IDaImofm ZDsa-ssnsntioa 3D1ittleansation 4Dnosttention 4. DowumadyukenyqedficnfiuypmsmingCaanSeanhymbckhgymnrwm domuanfimrepmthgmspidousmdividuflshohnprweyomafayfiomuimemcanpu? IDyes 2 Ciao 5. Anymawuedcanpunfitymmanpusmymbhefighttdephmmmy personnel)svailabletoyou? lClyes ZDao 6.1fyes,doyoueveruseanyoftbeaeservices? IUyes ZDno 7.1-Iownaicbdoyonwonysbombdigsvictimmcanpus? lClagrestdeal 20am 3Clslittle 4Ddon‘tworry 94 95 8. Ibwmchmflymnymacmfirym-hyumsmwhhdohgthhgsyoufiketo do? IDsgreatdeal 2Clsome 3Dsfltle 4C1aone 9. Proputyaimsbchdeshnglary,mothefi,mnddeuaaiuofprop¢ty. Howlikelydoyouthinkit thstyouwilbecomeadirectvictimofpropctyctinnwhielivhgucanpus? lflverEkely 2Ulikely 313m 4Uveryunlikely 10. WhndoyoumhkymnchncumofbehgavicthnofpmpatyahnsmaredmmhaNamh studanswholiveoncanpus‘? IDnaichgreater 2 Dgraster 3Uless 4Unascl|less 11.11aveyoueverbeuavictimofspropertyailns? 1Clyas 2Clno 12 anatpasaaalcrinehchdesmbbcy,sggrsvatsda.lh,smalasmkndnand¢. Howh'kelydoyou thhkhbthnymwmbecomsdheavictinofaviol-npmmwflefivhgmcanpus? lflvuyikely 20M “Unlikely 4Uvesyunlikely l3. andoywthhkyomdincumofbehgavicfimofafiolanpmdannecmaredmmha Nazarethstudanswboliveoncamus? lUMgreater 2 Dgrester 3Clleas 4Clnatchleas 14.1bwdepowdowubeiswtheConegehuwmymnmofbahgavicfimofsvbht persualcrimeucalmos’l lflagreatdeal ZUsome 30am 4Claone 15. Ibwnnchpowudoymbfieveymhvemremsceyowch-cuofbégsviainofavhlutpmd crineucaqms? lClsgteatdeal ZDsome 3C1sitle 4Daone 16. l7. l8. 19. 96 Ibwikdyhimnyoumflbesbhmphyialydehdymsdffiomumedviolatpmfl cr'mse? lflverylikely 201%er 3Clunfih1y 4Clverytmlikely Haveyoueverbeasvictimofsviolutpersmalcrime? 1Clyes ZDao Doyouknowofanyth'mgapcsoncoulddotoreduceln’sorharchancesofbehgavictimofsviolent personalcrimemcanpus? IDyes (pleasedesu'ire) 2C1no stymm-dednymmasdfidahnmuopdmgsnthecwegemdsewhae? “Eyes 20:10 Howssfedoyoufeelwhile... 20. 21. 22. ontbeNazarethCollsgecamusdm’ngdayliglnhous? IDverysafe 2Dsa£e 3Clunsafie 4Clverytmsafe oatheNanrabCollegecanpusaficdark? lflmsafe ” ZDsafe 30M 4Dvelyunsafe 'myourdorm? lClvelyaafe 2Clsa£e 3L'Juaaafie 4Clvelyunsafe How much do you agree with the following statements? 23. Mmqedficpmsapmcnuhwhiéwflrednmflsmhadnmofbahgsvicfinofs violutpcsoaalclineucanpns. lClagreevaynmcli ZUsgree BUdiaagree 4Udiaagreeverymch 97 24. SometmIesrmentamuedtommcauiinfstypmcsufionswhulamomwhhfiiuds lflsgreeverymnch ZUagree 3Ddisayee 4Ddiaagteeverymuch 25.1donotneedtotakesafstyprecsutioasmcanmua lClagreevesynarch 2C1agree 3Dd'nayee 4Udissgreeverynaich 26. Isometfinesdontukesafityprecsutionsbecmsetheytaketoonnchtime. lflsgreeverynatch ZDsgree 3Udiaagree 4Ddisagreeverynaich 27. Therein‘tmchspersoncmdotosvoidbehgavhtimofacrime. lClagreeverynasch 2Clagree BUd'nsyee _4Ddisagreeverynaich In general, how much of a hassle are the following safety precautions? 28. Askingsfiiudtowalkoncanpusw'zhyouafiadark 1Clveryeasy ZUeasy 3Clshassle 4U don'tdo 29. Callhgafiiadtowalkoncanpuswithyouafierdark IDveryeasy 2C|easy 3Dahaasle 4D don'tdo 30. CslbgCanpusSeanityforanescortacroascanpusafierdark 1Clvuyeasy” ZUeasy 3C|s1|sss1e 4C1 doa‘tdo 31. Avoid'mgpoorlyfitpsthsoncanpus IUvaryeasy 2Cleasy 3Usliaaale 4C] don‘tdo 32. bek'ngdonnroomatnight 1Clveryeasy 2Cleasy 3Uahassle 4Ddon'tdo 98 33. Keepigdormroomw’ndomlockedatnight IDveryessy 2Cleasy 3Ushsde 4C1don'tdo 34. Closingexteriorreaidencehalldoorsyoufindproppedopm lflvesyeasy ZDaasy 3Ual|assle 4Ddon‘tdo 35. Arrangingforfirst-timedstestooccwinfiniliarmound'lgs ICIveryeasy 2Dsasy 3Ushasde 4Cldon'tdo 36. Avo’al'mglesvhgapartyalonew'lhsnewacmn’anaace IDveryeasy ZDeasy 3Dsl|asale 4Ddoa‘tdo 37. Nofify'ngsecnr'tyofnqidouspmsoncanpusorintheresidencehafl 1Clveryeasy 2Ueasy 3Uallaasle 4Udon‘tdo 38. Ibwmddoyoubeievefinnkigprecnflnsndnufionhqms2&37mreduuyomchmus ofbeiigavictimofviolatcrirneoncanpus? IDsgreatdeal 2Clsome 3Uslittle 4Clnone This month,willyou... 39. call/asksfiiudtowalkwithyoueachtimeyoucrosscaqussfierdark? 1(3me ZUprobsblywill 3Dprobablywon't 4Ddeh'aelywon‘t 40. callCanpusSscmityforansacmtsaditimeyoucrosscanpnsafisrdark? 10mm 2Clprobablywill BDprobsblywoa't 4Udefifitelywon't 41. svoidpooslyhpatissadnimeyoucsoucanpusaficdark? 1134mm ZDptobsblywill 3Dprobablywon‘t 4Ddefin'nelywon‘t 42. 43. 45. 46. 47. 48. lockdormrooaneachnight? 113mm ZUprobablywill 3Dprobsblywun‘t 4Ddefiiitelywon‘t keepdormroomwindowslockedeachnight? lfldefiliclywill ZUprObablywill BUprobablywm't 4Udefiiitelywon‘t .closesproppeddooreachtimeyoufiidme'myourdosm? lfldefinitelywill 213mm 3D’probablywcn‘t 4Ddefinitelywon‘t arrangefisreachdstetoocaninfimilinnnrmmhgaifyouweretodstesomeoneforthefimfime? 1Ddefi'za1ywil 2mebab1ywill 3Dprobablym‘t 4Ddefinitelywoa‘t svoidlssviigeadiputyahoewihaaewscquainaacefi‘youwuetoauadapany? lCldefii'selywill szrobablywil 3Clprobab1ywon‘t 4Ddefinitelywon‘t aotifyucuiyifyonseeaniciouspmoamsor'nthemsideacehafls? 1C] deh'aelywill ZUprobablywill 3Clprobablywoa‘t 4Ddefinitelywon‘t DoympouecsSavhyEscmBepaohihedfiomtheCanpusSafayDepmmyommus? ICIyes ZUno Ifyeadoyoucnrrotlycarrythe SecurityEseas-tleeper? 1Clallthetime ZDmouofthetime 3Daotveryofien 4Clnever 49. Ovmlhowefiaah'emflymuythnthesmwleepckhpmufigahnemm? 1Clveryefi'ective 2Dsomewhstefi'ective 3C]aotverye&ctive 4Unotstal|efiective 50. WhnhdofimsadouSmRyWSysu-mumfifigsofpmdufuymms? lflmkesmefeelslotsafer 2C1 mkesmefielsomewhstsafer 3Uhashsdlittleinpsctonmyfechgsofpersoaalsafety 4C1makesmefeelhasaafe 100 Demographic Information (for classification purposes only) 51. thtisyourgaider? lDfemale ZDmIe 52.1-Iowdoyoudescribeyourself? lflAmericanlndianorNativeAlaskan SUMeaticanAmericaaorChicano ZDAimAsianAmuicanorPscificlhder 6DPuertoRican 3DBlsckorAfiicanAmerican 7DIat'm/South/Central 4DWbite Amaricmorotherl'fispanic - 80W 53. Howoldareyou? years 54. thtisyourclassstand'mg? IDFreshm 4DSenior ZDSophomore 50mm 3Clhmior 600016 55. HowlughaveyoulivsdoaCanpus? 101.cothsn0ne8emester SDFoursemesters ZUOnesemester 6C1Fivesemesters 3DTwosemesters 7DSixsemeaers 4UI‘lneesunsaers 8DMorethnsix 56. Whidlcstegorybearepresentsyomgradepo’mtsvelage? IBIS-4.0 4010-25 2010-15 SDWZD 3C1 2.5-3.0 THANK YOU FOR COMPLETING THIS SURVEY APPENDIX B APPENDIX B UCRIHS APPROVAL son \\\\\ Nazareth THIS project EXPIRES: \\\“ College September 30, 1996 AUG - 8 1997 WRENEWALAPPUCATION ONEMONTHPl-‘IORTO ABOVEDATETOW Dear Student: Please help us with a comprehensive study of safety on college campuses today. The enclosed survey focuses on experiences and perceptions of personal safety on our Campus. This research is part of a student's doctoral work and your participation by completing this questionnaire is critical in providing information useful to study the significant impact of crime on campus conmmnities. It is my hope that the results from this questionnaire could help yield information useful in making Nazareth a safer place for all students. The questionnaire should take about fifteen minutes to complete. We need your questionnaire no later than October 7. 1996. A response from each of you is very important. As a small means of saying thank you, we will enter the names of all who return their survey by October 7th in a drawing for three separate packages for a $25 gift certificate to CIAO'S and two movie tickets at the Pittsford Plaza Cinema. Once you complete the survey, please complete the enclosed card marked "Dinner and a Show" and.drop this card off separately from the completed survey to the Office of Residential Life so that you are included in the raffle. Your participation in this survey is completely voluntary and.your responses to the questionnaire will be anonymous. .At no time will your name be connected with your responses. You indicate your voluntary agreement to participate in this study by completing and returning this questionnaire. You may withdraw from this study at any time. If you have questions regarding your rights as a participant in this research, you may contact David Wright at (517)355-2180. Thank you so nmch for completing the survey. The findings from the research will be shared with the Division of Student Affairs to be distributed to the campus community. If you have any questions, please contact me at 586-2525 x700. Sincerely, Joan Anderson Director of Residential Life P.S. Your completed survey may be placed in the box.marked "Campus Safety Survey" located in the Office of Residential Life or mailed back in the enclosed envelope. Don't forget to complete the "Dinner and a Show" card!!!! 101 APPENDIX C APPENDIX C DINNER 81 A SHOW Please enter my name In the drawing for movie tickets to Pittsford Plaza Cinema and dinner at ClaO's. I understand that I must turn in my completed CAMPUS SAFETY SURVEY to be included in the drawing. Name 00°93'09": phone number out-campus address Office of Residential Life Nazareth college 102 APPENDIX D APPENDIX D THE CAMPUS SAFETY SURVEY St. John Fhller College Fall, 1996 Mmleedasnnvsybyphdmm'rbyyurmmdmhwmnnflmdw pm.hmmmmm15m.mwwmuupmmwammwm ideaifiedmaiywaymthsstudyrport. Timkyoul 1. Somepeoplearequheconcanadabomheahh,wl|ibothersueaot Howrmtchsttentiondoyoupayto ’ yourheakh? lClslotofsnenion 20mm 31311th 4Unoattention 2.1-IownndisttuniudoyoupaytowahsaspmgraI-ofisducmus? lelotofsnenioa 20mm 30mm 4Dnosnention 3.1ngeneralbowmdiattafioadoyoupaytoyourperaualnfuyonmus? lDalotot‘sneatioa 20mm 3Dl‘at1esneation 4Daoaneation 4. Doywanrutlyuhnyqedfie-fetyprecnnimshfingSeauhyuconabckhgyom mmdwrnaflfineareponhguqicioushdividuab)miwmeymunfayfiommmmus? lUyes 2 Duo 5. Anymaweofanpusmfmymicuwmhcmiymbhufightmmmiy persoanalhvailsbletoyou? lClyes 2 Duo 6.1fyeadoyouevaesesayofthaaesalvices? lDyes 20110 7. Hownarchdoyouwonysbmnbe'ngavict'unoacanpus? lClagreatdeal ZDsome 3Clal'atle 4Dh'tworry 103 104 8. Phwmrchwmflymnyfiflmfirymnflymmhafauwihdohgthhgsywfiketo do? legreatdeal ZDsmne 3Dsl'atle 4Daone 9. Propertycrhne'mchidesblughry,amothefi,usou.nddearucfimofprop¢ty. Howlikelydoyouth'mkit tbstywwfllbecomsadfiectvicthnofpropertycrhnewflelivingmcanpus? leeryflely ZDMy 3Dunflaly 4Dveryunlikely 10. WhndoywmhkymchncumofbehgsvicfinofpmaineconpuedeaSJPCnudmrs whofiveoncanpus? anaichgi-astsr 2 Elm 3Dless 4Dnaichless 11. Haveyweverbeusvictimofspropertycrime? lDyes 2 Duo 12. Violmtpasonaluinehchdesmbbay,aggnvnedasnuh,samalsmsuhndmder. Howlikelydoyou thhkhisthnymtwmbecomeadheavicfimofsvblutpmduinewhflefivhgmcanpus? 1Dverylikely 2 Dlikely 3Duaflaly 4Dverytmlikely 13. WhadoywthhkueymucbncuofbehgaviainofaviokntpasoadahneconpandmmherSFJC studentswholiveoncarmus? leichgrester 2Clgrester 3Clless 4Dnatcbless 14. HownaicbpowerdoyoubelievemeCollegehsstoreduceyomchancesofbe'mgsvictimofsviolent personalctimeoncamus? 1Dagreatdeal 2Dsorne 3Dal’ltle 4Dnoue 15. Howmdpowudoywbefieveymhvemremweywrcbucesofbdgsvicfimofavhhtpmd crimeoocanpus? 1Dsgrestdea1 2Dsome 3Dsl’ltle 4Daone Willi Inna—a." 105 16. Howikdyhhmnymwmflbesbbmphyicaflydehdymseflfimnmedviolatpmfl cr'nne? 10mm 20M 30m 40mm 17. Hsveyoueverbeenavictimofsviol-npcst-alcrime? lDyes 2Dno 18. Doyouknowofanythingapasoncoulddotoreduceh'norherchancesofbeingsvictimofsviolent personalcrimeoncanmus? lDyes (pleasedescribe) 2Dno 19. Haveywmudednymammmmrkmwdmeefingsnthecwegemdnwhm? 1Dyes 2Dno How safe do you feel while . . . 20. onthe St. John Fisher Collegecanmusdtn'hgdsylighthom’s? leerysafe 2Dsafe 3Dunsafe 4Dveryunsafe 21. onthe St. Job Fisher College csnpusafier dark? leerysafe 2Dsafe 3Dunsafe 4Dverytmsafe 22. 'myourdonn? leerysafe 2Dsafe 3Duasafe 4Dveryunsafe How much do you agree with the following statements? 23. Mmmedficprecsufimsapmmthichwflredwehkmhachmcuofbehgsvicfimof violentpersualcrimeoncanpua 1Dagreeverynmch 2Dagree 3Ddisagree 4Ddisagreeverynaich 106 24.8mmmmmam-hymm1amomwhhfiimds 1Dagleeverynatch 2Dagree 3Ddisagree 4Ddisagreeverynatch 25.1doaotaaedtotakeaafstyprecamionsoncaqus. lDagreevarynmch 2Dagree 3Ddissgree 4Ddisagreevelynaich 26. Isometimesdon'ttakesafatyprecsutionsbecaussthsytahstoonaichfime. 1Dsgreeverynasch 2Dagree 3Ddisagree 4Ddisagleeverynaich 27.1'hereim'tnaichapma-dotoavoidbdgsvictimofacsime. legreeveryrmch 2Dagree 3Ddisagree 4Ddisagreeverynaich In general, how much of a hassle are the following safety precautions? 28. Ask'ngafiimdtowalkoncanpuswithyouaficdark 1Dveryeasy 2Deasy 3Dshasale 4D don'tdo 29. Call'mgafiiudtowakucanpuswithyouafierdark leeryeasy 2Deasy 3Dahasale 4D don'tdo 30. CalliigCanpusSeciuityforanescortscmsscanpusafierdark 1Dve'yeasy' 2Deasy 3Dshasa|e 4D don'tdo 31. Avoidigpoorlyl'npathsoacanpus 1Dveryeasy 2Deasy 3Dahanle 4D drm'tdo 32.1.aock'ngdormroomstaight lesryeasy 2Deasy 3Dahassle 4Ddon‘tdo 33. 34. 35. 36. 37. 38. 107 Ksep'mgdormroomw'mdowslocksdstaight 1Dveryaaq 2Deasy 3Dshade 4Ddon‘tdo leeryeasy 2Deasy 3Dshsde 4Ddon‘tdo Arranghgfinfirst-timedstestooccm'mfaniliarnnroud’mgs leeryeasy 2Deasy 3Dahade 4Ddon‘tdo Avoid'mglsav'mgspartyalonewihaaewscqus'anaace leeryeasy 2Deasy 3Dahssale 4Ddon'tdo Notifyiigsecmityofatqiiciouspcsonsoncanpusorhtheresiducehan leeryeasy 2Deasy 3Dshassle 4Ddon'tdo mechdowubehevemnukhgpmsmdiuthueaions2&37cmmywchmces ofbeiigsvictimofvioluncrimeoacanpus'? legreatdeal 2Dsome 3Dal‘atle 4Dnone Thismonth,willyon... 39. call/askafi'iadtowalkw'nhyousachtimeyoucrosscanpusafierdsrk? lDdefinitelywill 2Dprobablywill 3Dprobablywon’t 4Ddefin'aelywon't 40. callCmssSecur’lyforanasconaachtimeyoucmascanpusafierdark? 1(3me 20mm 3Dprobablywm't 4Ddefifitelywon't 41. avoidpooriylitpathseachtimeymicrosscanmusaficdark? 1:1me 213me summit 4Ddeb'aelywon‘t 42. 43. 45. 47. 48. 49. 50. 108 Mdosmroomeadaight? 1Ddefm'zalyvu‘ll 2Dprohsblywl 3Dpsohsblywont 4Ddefinitelywon't keepdormroomw'ndowslockedsachaight? lDdefin'zelywm 2Dprobablywill 3Dprobablywon‘t 4Ddefii'nelywon't .closesproppeddooreachtimeyoufmdme'nyomdorm? 113mm 2Dprobablywil 3Dprohablywon‘t 4Ddefinitelywon‘t mgeforuddnemocmhmmdigah‘ywwcemdnemformefirnfime? 1D dehitelywfll 2D probablywil 3Dprobdilywm‘t 4D dehitalywm't .svoidhsvhgeschputyahnewihsaewscquaiatuceifyouwuetosumdsputy? 11:]me 2Dptobablywill 3Dprobablywm‘t 4D definitelywon‘t nofifysecmiyifyouseeanispidouspmoncanmusorhthereddmcehans? 1Ddehitelywill 2Dprobab1ywill 3Dprohab1ywon't 4D definitelywon‘t Doyoucm-rutlycarryahandheldpersonalsafetyalarm? 1Dyes 2Dno Howeflecfiwdoyouthhkhndheldpasmflnfuyalumswwflbehmmumus? leeryefiaive 2Dsmnswhstefi‘ective 3Dnotveryefiv‘ective 4Dnotatallefl‘ective Whakhdofhmaadoymfihkhudhddpamflmfayahmmflhwmymnfeebgsofpmsl safetyoncama? 1Dwouldmkemefeelslotsafsr 2Dwouldnlkemefselsomewhatsafir 3D wouldhsvel'atlehmiactonmyfeeligsofpuaoaalsafuy 4Dwouldmakemefsellesssafe 109 Demographic Information (for classification purposes only) 51. thtisymtrgender? lDfsmale 2Dnnle 52. Howdoyoudescribeyoursell’? lDAmericanlndianorNstiveAlsskan SDMexicanAmericsnorChicano 2DAsian,An'anAmsricanorPadficlslander 6DPnertoRicaa 3D'BlackorAfiicanAmerican 7D Latin/South/Central 4DWhite WormherI-l‘upanic 8D0ther 53. Howoldsreyou? years 54. Whatisyourdassnaad'ng? lDFredumn ' 4DS¢|ior ZDSophosnose 5DGsaduateStudut 3Dhmior 6DOthsr 55.1-10wlonghsveyoufivedonCanmus? lDLessthaaanSenssaer SDFoursameners 2DOnesemester 6DI-‘ivesemesters 3D1‘wosemsners 7DSixsemeaers 4D'1‘hreesesnesters 8DMorethansix 56. Whichcategorybestrepresatsyompadepo'utsvenge? 1D 3.5-4.0 2D 3.0-3.5 3D 2.5-3.0 4010-23 5Duader2.0 THANK YOU FOR COMPLETING THIS SURVEY APPENDIX E UCRIHS APPROVAL FOR THIS prqact’ EXPIRES: AUG - 8 1997 WRENEWALAPPLEATION mammro September 30, 1996 ABOVEOATETOOONTNUE Dear Student: Please help us with a comprehensive study of safety on college campuses today. The enclosed survey focuses on experiences and' perceptions of personal safety on our Campus. This research is part of my doctoral work and your participation by completing this questionnaire is critical in providing information useful to study the significant impact of crime on campus consmmities. It is my hope that the results from this questionnaire could help yield information useful in making SJFC a safer place for all students. The questionnaire should take about fifteen minutes to complete. We need your questionnaire no later than October 7. 1996. A response from each of you is very inportant. As a small means of saying thank you, we will enter the names of all who return their survey by October 7th in a drawing for three separate packages for a $25 gift certificate to CIAO'S and two movie tickets at the Pittsford Plaza Cinema. Once you complete the survey. please complete the enclosed card marked "Dinner and a Show" and drop this card off separately from the completed survey to the Office of Residential Life so that you are included in the raffle. Your participation in this survey is completely voluntary and your responses to the questionnaire will be anonymous. At no time will your name be connected with your responses. You indicate your voluntary agreement to participate in this study by completing and returning this questionnaire. Youmay withdraw from this study at any time. If you have questions regarding your rights as a participant in this research, you may contact David Wright at (517) 355-2180. Thank you so such for completing the survey. The findings from the research will be shared with the Division of Student Affairs to be distributed to the campus commity. If you have any questions. please contact me at 385-8230. Sincerely, Mary-Beth Cooper Dean of Students P.S. Your completed survey may be placed in the box marked "Campus Safety Survey" located in the Office of Residential Life or mailed back in the enclosed envelope. Don't forget to complete the "Dinner and a Show" card!!! 110 APPENDIX F APPENDIX F Office of Residential Life St. John Fisher college DINNER & A SHOW Mementermynamelnthedrawinorormovleticketsto Pittsford Plaza Cinema and dinner at ClaO's. I understand that I must turnlnmycompletedCAIlPllSSAFETYSURVEYtobelncludedin thedrawlno. 111 APPENDIX G APPENDIX G Don’t FOrget... To complete your campus safety survey and turn it into the office of Residential Life. If you misplaced your original survey, you can get another from your RA. Also, don't forget to enter the "Dinner and A Show" drawing. 112 APPENDIX H 517155-21!) PM 51733-1171 APPENDIX H MICHIGAN STATE 1 lJ hi I \' E I! S 1"Y August 12. 19s: '10: -II 24 Hill . Pittsford. I? 1453s - rm : 9‘ -413 ‘3' rrrts: ‘nnr erstriowssrr or srunssr vsecrrriows AID ssnsvroes ssoannrso erase-st SAIlTY: A consanarrvr sruuv or run SHALL snrvarr corneas RIVISION IIDUISTID: IVA TIOOII: 2-1 APPIOVIL Dill: OI/OCI" it coaldttee on nesearch Invol nuaan huh ects'tuc3f38) gazigeizfrghiz project is coaplete., I aa.;§:2sed to advise that the rights and welfare of the huaan subjects appear to be adequately and chads to obtain infor-ed consent are a riate. ¥§:::§:;:. theuac1138 approved this project and any rev :ggns listed IIIIIIL: UCIIIB is valid for one calendar . inn with the appgggigvaite shown above. Investigatg::rpl::ging12g continue a project one year lust use the green renewal form (enclosed with original a roval letter or when a 2:23.Ct is renewed) to seek te certification. There_is a one of four such expedit renewals ssihle. Investigators wish to continue a project beyond the tine need to submit it again or complete rev ew. esvrsrosss UCIIIB'Iust review :2! changes in gzocedures involving huaan subjects, prior to tiation of change. If this is done at the ties o , please use the renewal form. To revise an roved protocol at angcg r tine during the year. send your w: tten request to the 135 Chair. requesting revised :gproval and r:f:rencé:g ghznprgject's 128 8 and title.. Igclude reques des o the change and an revise ins ruaents. consent forge or advertise-ants that {re applicable. should either of the fell arise during the course of the work. investigators oust noti ucnrns promptly: (1) roblees unexpected side effects coup aints. etc.) involving suhgectzig: 52) ghgnges :he r=::a:ch environ-enabor new inf rue indi ing grea er r o the human s 'ects than existed when the protocol was previously reviewed and approved. If we can he of future hel 1.... do no at (517)35: :iac.g¥ rar (517;; §-§171, ‘ h"*"" ‘0 Goat‘s: n- 0 vid I. wright. Ph OCIIIB Chair ”shod cc: Kathryn M. Hoore 113 APPENDIX I APPENDIX I March 19 , 1996 Dr. Tully Lenski Dean of Students Trinity College of Vermont 208 Colchester Ave Burlington. VT 05401 Dear Dr. Lenski. I am requesting permission to use the Campus Safety Survey you developed in 1991. I am a doctoral student at Michigan State University and I am proposing to investigate the relationship of student perceptions and behaviors regarding personal safety, at two small. private colleges in the Northeast. I would like to use the Campus Safety Survey with a few additional items on parent a peer influence on safety issues. as well as student reported use of personal safety devices. It is my intention to test the usefulness of the Safety Belief Model in predicting students' behavioral intentions. to examine the impact of personal safety devices on behavior and to investigate the influence of parents and peers on personal safety issues. The use of the Safety Survey will be solely for the purposes of coagulation of my degree. I will credit you fully in my writing and only alter the instrument as stated above. If you would be interested in my findings. I would be happy to send you information after my research is complete. thank you for your attention to this matter. I look forward to hearing your response. Mary-Beth Cooper 24 Wood Hill Rd. Pittsford. NY 14534 114 APPENDIX J APPENDIX J OFVElaOfl 0.»de March 21, 1996 Mary-Beth Cooper 24 Wood Hill Road Pittsford. NY 14534 Dear Mary-Beth. ThankwaorycurnqueutonsetheCampuSafetySuncyinyourdoctonlworkat Michigan State University. Understand thatyou wish to modify the instrument to includeaddifionalvarisbles. Youhavemypennissimtodoso. I'mverypleasedyw‘ve foundtheinsuumeatandtheSafetyBeliefModelhelpfulinyourwork. laminterenedinthemmeofyourresesrchandwmldlovetohearhomywwhen you’re done. Safety behavior research is much lauded to help colleges develop useful safetypropmsforsmdenuandworklikeyounandminewillhelp-avoidin availableinfoxmation. lwishywthebestasyoucompleteyourresearch. llS BIBLIOGRAPHY American Council on Education (1985, reissued 1987). . Washington, DC, One Dupont Circle. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. ED265774). Astin, A. (1977). Egnz_Czinical_Xears. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass Publishers. Bausell, C.R., Maloy, C.E., & Sherrill, J.M. (1989). Ihe ' ° . MD: Towson State university Campus Violence Prevention Center. Becker, M.H., Maiman, L.An, Kirscht, J.P., Haefner, D.P., & Drachman, R. H. (1977). The health belief model and prediction of dietary compliance. A field experiment. W. 18. 348- 366 Biglan, A., Metzler, C., Wirt, R., Ary, D., Noell, J., Ochs, L., French, C. and Hood, D. (1990). Social and behavioral factors associated with high- -risk sexual behavior among adolescents. Journa1_cf_neha2ioral Medicine. 11. 24S— 262 Burger, J. M. and Burns, L. (1988). The illusion of unique invulnerability and. the use of effective contraception. ' , 11, 264- 270. Castelli, J. (1990). Campus crime 101. Heu_xsz_Iimes, pp. 1-2. Cockey, M. Sherrill, J. M. & Cave, R. E. II. (1989). Towson State university research on campus violence. In J.M. Sherrill, & D. G. Siegal (Eds. ). RespondinLtuiclencem campus (pp. 17-28, 103-106). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass Publishers. College and University Law Enforcement Officers and Campus Housing Administrators (Producers). (1985). Ihe WW facilities. Louisville, KY: Campus crime prevention workshop. Cummings, K.M., Jette, A.M., & Rosenstock, I.M. (1978). Construct validation of the health belief model. Health We. 5. 394-405. 116 117 Dolcini, M.M., Cohn, L.D., Adler, N.E., Millstein, S.G., Irwin, C.E., Kegeles, S.M., and Stone, G.C. (1989). Adolescent egocentrism and feelings of invulnerability: Are they related? WW. 9.. 409- 418. Evans, N.J. (1983). Environmental assessment: Current practices and future directions. .Jnurnal_nf_Cnllene StudenLPersnnnel. 2.4. 293-299. Fesenthal, E. (1994, November 29). More colleges are divulging crime reports. Ihe_flall_snzeen_innzndl. pp. Bl. B10. Fox, J. A. & Hellman, D. A. (1985). Location and other correlates of campus crime W. 13, 429- 444. Glanz, R., Lewis, F.M., & Rimer, B.K. (Eds.) (1990). Health practice. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass Publishers. Gray M. (1990). The effectiveness of personalizing acquaintance rape prevention: Programs on perception of vulnerability and on risk-taking behavior. .qurnal_cf Cnllene_Student_Dexelnnnnnt. 31. 217—220. Greene, E. (1988, September 28). Pressed by students and parents colleges step up their efforts to fight crime on campuses. Winn. 15(5). A1. A31. Hanchette (1988). The Clerys: Victimls Parents Push Change. W, October 4, 1988, p. 6A. Harris, D. M. & Guten, S. (1979) Health-protective behavior: An exploratory study. Journal_nf_nealth_and_3ncial Belmvinr. 2.0. 17- 29. (1990, March 14). Washington, DC: Hearing before the Subcommittee on Postsecondary Education of the Committee on Education and Labor, House of Representatives. (Eric Document Reproduction Service No, ED 325 011) Kingston, R., Strunin, L., and Berlin, B. (1990). Acquired immunodeficiency syndrome transmission. Changes in knowledge and behaviors among teenagers, Massachusetts statewide surveys, 1986- 1988. Pediatrics. £5. 24- 29. 118 Hirschorn, M. W. (1987, September 2L A parental crusade to force Colleges to reveal crime data. Chrnnicle_nf_Hisher Educatinn. Pp. A3, A28. Hochbaum, G. M. (1958). Public participation in medical screening programs. A.sociopsychological study. Public HealtLSeanmhlicatinn. no. 572. Jessor, R. (1984). Adolescent development and behavioral health. In J. D. Matarazzo, S. M. Weiss, J. A. Herd, N. E. Miller, a S. W. Weiss (Eds ). Beharinral_nealth (pp. 69- 89). New York. John Wiley 5 Sons. Jessor, R. (1991). Risk behavior in adolescence: A psychosocial framework for understanding and action. W11. 12. 597-605 . Jessor, R. (1992). Risk behavior in adolescence: A psychosocial framework for understanding and action. W. 12. 374-390. Kasl, S.V., & Cobb, S. (1966). Health behavior, illness behavior, and sick- role behavior. Encircnmental_nealth. 12, 246-266. Kirscht, J. P. Haefner, D., Kegeles, S. S. Rosenstock, I. (1966). A national study of health beliefs. innrnal_n£ Win. 1. 242- 254 Kosimar in Kobasic, D.M., Smdth, E.R. s Barmore-Zucker, L.S. (1988). Eisman v. State of New York: The duty of a college to protect its students from harm by other students admitted under social programs. Ihe_flnnrnal_n£ Cnllene_and_Unixersitx_Lau.1.4. 575- 605. Larson, E. B. Olsen, E., Cole, W., and Shortell, S. (1979) The relationship of health beliefs and a postcard reminder to influenza vaccination Jnurnal_nf_£amil¥_2ractice. 8. 1207- 1211. Lederman, D. (1995). Colleges Report Rise in Violent Crime. ' ' , February 3, 1995, A31, A42. Lenski, T.J. (1992). W Unpublished manuscript. ACUHO-I Research and Educational Foundation, Columbus, OH. Lenski, T.J. (1992). W Unpublished doctoral dissertation, university of Ferment. 119 Lenski, T. J. Meyers, E. W. , a Hunter, D.E. (1996). Understanding Students' Intentions to Use Safety Precautions. ' ' AdministratanJnurnal. 31.82- -93 Maiman, L. A. s Becker, M. H. (1974). The health belief model. Origins and correlates in psychological theory. WW. 2. 336- 353. Maiman, L.Am, Becker, M.H., Kirsht, J.P., Haefner, D.P., & Drachman, R.H. (1977). Scales for measuring health belief model dimensions: A test of predictive value, internal consistency, and relationships among beliefs. Health W. 5.. 215- 231. Maslom A.H. (1954). Win}; New York: Harper 1 Row. Moore, 8., and Rosenthal, D. (1991). Adolescent invulnerability and perceptions of AIDS risk. innznal_ni AdolescenLResearch. 5. 164-180 . Ordovensky, P. (1990, November 29). Hour by hour, campus crime toll mounts. HSA_cha¥, p.8A. Orr, D. P. Beiter, M., and Ingersoll, G. (1991). Premature sexual activity as an indicator or psychosocial risk. 2cd11£rin&..fil. 141- 147. Pace, C.R. (1984). An Account of the Development and Use of the College Student Experiences Questionnaire. unpublished manuscript. Higher Education Research Institute, Graduate School of Education. University of California, Los Angeles. Pascarella, E.T., & Terenzini, P.T. (1991). ch_ccllece it I . . . . . nf_research. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass Publishers. Roark, M. L. (1987). Preventing violence on college campuses. WM. 55. 367- Roark, M., & Roark, E. Jr. (1987). Administrative Responses to Campus Violence. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the American College Personnel Association/National Association of Student Personnel Administrators. Chicago, IL, March 15-18, 1987. Robertson, L. S. (1977). Car crashes: Perceived vulnerability and willingness to pay for crash protection. W. 3. 136- 141. 120 Robinson, B.E. (1988). .Teenage_£athers. Lexington, MA Lexington Books. Rosenstock, I.M. (1974). Historical origins of the health belief model. Health_Educatinn_Mnnnnranhs. 2. 328-335. (a) Rosenstock, I. M. (1974) The health belief model and preventive health behavior. nea1th_Edncaticn_Mcncgraphs, 2. 354-385. (b) Rosenstock, I.M. (1990) The health belief model: Explaining health behavior through experiences. In R. Glanz, F.M. Lewia. & 8.x. Rimer (Eds.). Health_behaxinr_and_health__ educat1nn1_Ihenrxi_researchi_and_nrnct1ce (pp. 39-62). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass Publishers. Rudolph, (1990). ' University of Georgia Press, pp.87, 97. Schneider (1977). In.A. L. Rent: and G. L Saddlemdre (Eds.). WWW (pp 232- 233). Springfield, Illinois: Charles C Thomas Publisher. Simpson, J.A. (1994). Developing a Safety/Security Program For Residence Halls. In Camnns_Hnusinc (pp.4l-53). Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the Association of College and University Housing Officers-International. San Antonio, TX, June 26-30, 1994. Smith, M. C. (1989). The ancestry of campus violence. In J. M. Sherrill, s D. G. Siegal (Eds. ). Resnnndins_tn Iinlence_nn campus (pp. 5- 15), San Francisco. Jossey- Bass Publishers. Towson State University, Center for the Study and Prevention of Campus Violence (1986,1987,1988).Nhn1nnal_camnnn We Towson, MD: Towson State University. Towson State University, Center for the Study and Prevention of Campus Violence (1990). Ihc_11nkS_amOng_drn91_dldnhnli Wm WWW 21ct1mizatinn_suryey. Towson, MD: Towson State University. Turner, J. (1993). WM .1 l'J'I°Tl'EJ El] .. Doctoral dissertation, The University of Connecticut, 1993. 121 Tuttle, D.F. (1990). In S. Colino (author), Felony 101. StudentJamcer. p. 37. united States Congress (1990L Ihe.£hnden£_319hnchncxnnn Washington, DC. U. S. Government Printing Office. Weinstein, N. D. (1980). Unrealistic optimism.about susceptibility to health problems. anurnal_nf_2ersnnalitx ancLSnciaLPsxchnlncx. 39.. 806- 820. Weinstein, N. D. (1982). Unrealistic optimism about susceptibility to health problems. Medicine. 5. 441- 460. White. R.W. (1975). W naturalJrnutanfmrsnnalitx (3“ d ). New York: Holt, e . Rinehart & Winston, 1975. Wills. 8.3. (1993). WWW BSIII :2 I IT 21].“. .|.. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Illinois State university. "‘uumu