.h...+,,,_ _. . .... . ...flwqu..§a1 w. .. ..t._m-.w:£“wo.,..... ..L..\.c.- fi .14." ‘ ...I. o Kr%;..%¢m§ an . r: 3.. ...-wanna...” .. ...-m . ... . . . . . 1 a...» ......i : .-....um-u. 54-. .2: -..-anew? .. ..fi 13; .. 4......- ilflw... :6: . . ....u- rum-M. .mmw...-.-......ww§a.=.w, ...-m. y...- n. i. - . . quwfiafifinfir we. _ .-.-mug. . gown-Wu ...-km».- , fist-13.”... ...TT . . . ... z . ...:phk ... ...... 3%? _ . ...?ik... ...-.... gnaw... a... .... .... w... ' "X' . Fa r- "a... 2%....fi . . m. .11. Ana-v... . ._ N5... ....- v- I. v! _ u I" ... - . ...-#5....- .31.- I- . a . uni... uwyfimwan ..- . . ...1 - .1. Mg,- 111..-; .....- . . . .9"! . .. ...fialfliifi ... » .IKIILH-Rrv 3!.de 4.5.5- NFL-nan...”- . - DC.‘ Oil-rt Iv. .9. ... . - .- . - luau-«1:5? Afl$ . arr-n ,.. .fi-k-WW-E-pfl: .FS : . ..vflw. ..n-N ...mW-h-hw-xfi... . Ada-4mm? ....-.u......,. nwame-fififidflfi. u. ll ...-.11. .3. Lip) .. . n. . < . . flfin ... ....-...-.-. - . S had....9-uui-§.fi-1Pv.1.c .- . .. ._ . .u- ...-..-3.r.u..-..r-..-- 95.41%. a... .65.“an .... Cf...»- .. . . . . . z --.. v ... . hi... ......ub....4t-3..3 ......z. .. ....EJ-u-F-hv- t .l- tit-hu- ! ..a-u-mfim 5.9....»- nmhvdv ......M bun-...- ..-....- I. Sum-u... L 52.3- 69.3“. . . hum. ..Mu..rivn--Rz ,- ai-C:-f;a..1-.-4ul-.. u..- --.-..-- . $.24.- . QM"... 2- Gun-- 5...... in... 511)., fi. . 33- Ir! - 4dr? o..Lu»-.Ipv.o.....3 .\ ...-lawmflnndfixc 3.321... W. hahffian ”Gui...- -u..l..¢..h.l-Ys.?.r§..rc . ‘s-0 .1 I I- O. ‘A I O ...-I. 5 l'hIQI-u.‘l .‘I .Ma-wuflmhuwm-Wm. - ..wmodmmflxi-a-un - £48... .. -.:..r.. - . - .. then-.... .... c.13- to. - . . =25; . ..3. . .9... fl. . ...-115.3... (-91-. - ... . . .v. . . .12? 1d? . 15-5 ... .5 2-19., -. . i..-h-u...-.In.£m-.u- ...... ...-h -.. .Nn..fi-H-..;r a gun-nu-w-«xs-iqiéi; .. - . .nb .... . z - -. 1...-....n..-...(x - ...-J. a ...-..- ..1 k... .. «1L..- wz-a- . . , .. -.. ...-.... .1... )7 $lV-J. l . 1L4 In"! (nib! VI~vI 501.11 1 ...-...f- .- .I?..l01h(.u , 4 ....Qk'.tv“dvc\10 ‘14». pgfiflka .0!!!- . . 1.- . - . - . .. .. 1., .11.. ..i- . ,l ..... #3 ..I 3 «....o. ,. .34- .m- -.. -.. ...» .7. . ...-3 . .. ...-ht. .1- ui- -.. :- . . . ...-.....skdnl__i.-. 1. - 5...... ...d . 1.5.4... 189-... .- ...-...“. -..-i: . Jun: -1; :10 . 1...... .1 4!- . ...-3... . . .... Ianl!‘ . P \. ... I- tn :fii-I-Ifw rt! 3!...- ..I u!.. .. ..., ’1'.» 1c- Az-bvaa :- . . trot ..l. .14 .-I . I... ..- Eli-1 «é. y - ...-.31... .- 93-..- -. _ - ..-.(..n-.- .271... , .. . {ah-7!.- «.- ..... t {...- ...-...... ... 1.4%....- .-.-.-...I.-- . .. 2... 413.311.. . 4 art-It. I! . ... . .. v‘ll v3 v1 l1. .. l. .. 'i I ...-C II I '- v..t-t.lIl-qv . .11. ~ I. r XII-il‘lt-r... -I-A2I In...» 111...: IS- 911' .11.. . I ’1 ill?! c IIIII I a #t..vv%t..l.~! - -- 1.1.13.1.- . .-I1Ivn~4..lul| p all '1 t» id'- . -- - . ,- .- ..-; ...-2 1:. $5 1- :...i. . . 1.1-... . {Ii-11¢ :- - . 9- ..rklr; flvr6.‘§? ., AVE-l. .. I. IDInWOer‘il ain‘t utilvl‘vtfifiu‘l» tau“ . . .. wovtxz-Ol-DuOu \ :r’lS‘lu . v . O .. r-Ll-huu. l.- hfihfir 6..........F.c:....v..--- 4...“... ...-...r. . . §.1-.-.1-Ivl§1-l- - (nonna- ii- 1.- .2'.l-Iv ..- ..... 11 n'....... 11-36..- . :II :1: - .lI ......... ll! 11-1- P. In! 1 L." Italvl-ll-‘Lr. w‘l‘v .. 1!... 11:7 . V.- v . . vol-l A: -.¢......mu-..v#-.--..--:u.-nv ...-$1...- .Jfiuu... . . . {lbw-m1}... .... 1....- -. .-. v F:§mflh§h-¥§ ,- 1.1-;- . . . .. . . oil-io- tl'lv). . tfiufldur-i inn-u, 5-1.1}: 5-1.: ... a. . {flu-.- .. .. . . . 313-.“- é-lvr-v [...-.1 ... i- at It". a A ...-v. 2...? i-rI .1 '. . V . . . . -.. . - . . . . 7|,1‘0-I'A a. D‘l. . tic-III: I Elli-... ..lc- .. . . .. 5.. - .tl‘u-v .- - . I . 51!"; v. . .... ... .... .31.. ...- ...f- , - - ...-1.3- . 1 -rv- - Mann-11v. ...- . - . .1 -v 3- - -..-...: .. . . .. - ali‘r-VW - L0.Din).t . 1.7.1 - .(-x~v - . .. y a unit-<1 ‘90.- Iii-u «1...! - - .---...--- <1. . . . 31... . . .. 1..., . i I III"! 3’. V. IV. 0 - - u i u . Sll-tO'U'OL ’1 ‘l l . .g. ...-...... .y... . .. . . r . . . : ., . . . 1113-1-11.- . . \- .. ...-n 1......- 1-.-.. .. , . . . . . _ 91:. - . ,.-.o..lu);-I)Dbln- ...I n at . ~ . -. . . . . . . , . {-r ..l- IIVJON-v-Jf blx . ......” 141‘ Hal-:59)?- o . J . - I . z r . . -. II. n ’u If I ... )1 311-0 t-pa..l 57‘ a £9.15 7- l. . . . . - . . . J1\VlI'-ut 1- l.- IYII. 1' . b 115...»..- u - .... ...-315.. (.... .. . . . . .11..- I..-.t.-. .r-i. Lilia-Hull . . 5-. ..l-nu- ...-v- . a... 5! 92:12.... . . . . . . , : . . 1.2115111413131111. 1.. . . I.” Lat-fnuFU-‘zl 34.1.3111?! ark-unal- . . . . . . l I}: - . 3. 1111417.... - . ,. . .3- .. .. i .. .. - 1.27.37 . . . .. . ....o.!-.1.-:-l--.fl..-si1. :- ol! . . . . ~ [dunk-2.3!: 1- ...... . . . Vii-312.1. n. 19.5.... - .- ..h-l-(l! ...H-ulslmunui... {...-1.1». . . I.Ia‘:§ i n . v.1 I l\.KOa 9|- 1- ,623 31-1.! h...»- -- . 112.32.81.1- . {IA-.1- 31.- .- .. -..--.....--. th‘l II ‘19.“. .... 1": ’c?l, I‘M-Ehnuo’tv It). .I I ~ V In" 3"3‘hl-}~ all 33-1 .v 1.- - .-. .. .f ,z.. .2... . 2-. . - - - c- . ..- ...... .- . . I ..- . -. . 5-1;! - . . - . --.€-o....Jn.-IA:-.--.-.1u-..“a ...”.rma-‘yfi-ann-ffi -.-fi--.-£u-u-.-om-J..-§.r.--m5mw- {41.11215} inn-1411;. 1-...1-8-1Qt-ui-s-lz.“...... llWI-1711-112i-1- .-- . v I - o . 7:? D- ‘14): vugfl: )t‘II.-ul-' liO‘ 5-3.3:- . 9.5 b - u.- .. .. {...-.33. - ...-1.01.. . VB; I'GV-Ilhvl I! like.“ - plut' Judah“. Y5 I|I. 5‘.- [‘1‘ lb‘ . l- ...: 1 (- - -. . . ......- ...-...?95-31311 ; an... ...-.r 111.13. .. . .95.- I flulfiluhhffl. "Navy .. :l-un-l - ...Ifl'hnvI-v-J- urn. It} :o-l . 1| - 7| A $2.11!! 1,-1.- . .u... .‘vuxlew («J-7 w . ..- JJH mall-.... .- V y? Ab’OV'tt‘l l! 1 II .| lnzl‘t.‘ 07-1 H1 14. . .--.D‘t|rfi||N-anhll.- “AT-40.11, ..1} 41.1 lit-1v- 11: 1-11-11: ‘- . .-.. t ........ 11-11:}- .. o..u-a . . 21-0-1. «- II»- .. - Uhnv‘vh-n-WHD - .. . A LIIi-x- ‘lll-sl"-’1ll11l‘l1 -.-| ’1 0- . l ...-1.11! .ll..l1lf fl.“ I ll.) - 1.1-0... I.M-v-.-l-r .4 I. .)||-.1 v1.1! '1 A. I r»! In ~If1-II1' “'VHIIII‘ Ill-1.14!- u‘l’ ..vIIOV-VIJI- lulu-1141111111.,Ol 11'. Io!)|-Hl |OOL vl1- .v-..-. 0101‘!- l'.‘ $111!.r I! n; oft-d...- 199 1 l1! lit-Int" Jittl 1!- I.- - f-.. |- 1- 1 la] 1111-11 1 - lrtl l.- ..F 0 111I1.| {ll 1 -'!ll1-: lv-[II 111 -1«.\.- 1-114 -J| 011-01111) 111.32-111.11-1 I 17' nu-r......O--. 7...D-1---I|11-61 - l-1|-I1‘|l 01- 1 l- l.-v-1 V 1.‘ . I 2 .I.vl-l-II. .111). I‘ll-lf-II-r’ le’fa’ . {LI-11 1| ¥.l A! ~.v«-V.l-11-l-|III ‘i-ulli‘l . . .... FJ-f..1- . .. {LI-.... {If-1410111111111 3r . {-1 11.1 a... 10-1.! .-.l|1-?1-I|-F|-. 111-!) -l- . . . . A - ...-1:. of- . -- i1 Ill-(1.1; v 1|.llill’ll’ la .....ll-1«-.-1. . 111- l..- .....- . -.-1-clt11‘...-IL-l-l -1 1 . ..t‘ -!.| o.l'l‘l’il {til-v - VI- 1 - - - V . . ‘1' || - up: -‘ | .u i v 7- L- 'HESlS SlTYL LBI RABIES llllllllll‘llllllllllllllllllllllllllllllllllll| ll 3 1293 01570 This is to certify that the thesis entitled AN ASSESSMENT OF RISK PERCEPTION IN TRANSPORTING HAZARDOUS MATERIALS BY RAIL THROUGH DEXTER VILLAGE, MICHIGAN. AND ”Imsmfi‘ééeé’t‘éfiooy BETHANNMYERS has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for M-A- degree in Geoqraphy Wei/iv 233333;, Major NU essor Date EKKQO [C] r) 0-7639 MS U is an Affirmative Action/Equal Opportunity Institution LIBRARY Mlchlgan State ‘ Unlverslty ‘ PLACE ll RETURN BOX to roman this chockout from your record. . TO AVOID FINES Mum on or before date duo. DATE DUE DATE DUE DATE DUE l_ - - [:r—J I ISL—J l___Jl——l- L____ l—W—‘T—j MSU Is An Affirmative Action/Equal Opportunlty lnstltwon AN ASSESSMENT OF RISK PERCEPTION DNTFRAUQSPCHITHNCI HAZARDOUS MATERIALS BY RAIL TTflRCflJCfli IHiXTIflIVHLlJUEE,LflKHIKlAbI 3AA“) thMNHSEKHfl83CHIK) by Beth Ann Myers A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fiilfillment of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF ARTS Department of Geography 1 997 ABSTRACT AN ASSESSMENT OF RISK PERCEPTION 1N TRANSPORTING HAZARDOUS MATERIALS BY RAIL THROUGH DEXTER VILLAGE, MICHIGAN AND MIAMISBURG, OHIO by Beth Ann Myers Recognition of hazardous materials in manufacturing and recognition of the disposal risks has been acknowledged in past literature, but little research has addressed the intermediary step of hazardous materials transportation. This thesis is concerned with identifying risk perceptions in conjunction with the transportation of hazardous materials by rail. A survey questionnaire was distributed in the study areas, Dexter Village, Michigan, and Miamisburg, Ohio, and then evaluated for risk perception levels and demographic characteristics of the respondents. Statistical analysis including regression and t-tests were incorporated to ascertain whether certain demographic variables can be indicative of one's risk perception level. The major finding of this study, which parallels past perception literature, is the higher risk perception levels of female respondents. Other comparisons between the two study areas and the remaining demographic variables were inconclusive. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS I would like to thank the following people for making my Master’s experience rewarding and this thesis a lesson in tenacity. First of all, Dr. Pigozzi for his encouragement, patience, and unwavering support throughout this process. Dr. Burley and Dr. Olson for their comments, suggestions, and help with the final thesis. My husband, Jeffrey, for his help in all phases of this thesis. And lastly, my family for their continued encouragement and support during my tenure at Michigan State University. iii TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES LIST OF FIGURES INTRODUCTION AND BACKGROUND Structure of this Paper RELEVANT LITERATURE The New Environmental Paradigm Risk and Experts vs. Laypeople Factors Influencing Risk Perception Perceptions Concerning Transporting Hazardous Materials Regulations METHODS Research Design Study Area Miamisburg, Ohio Dexter Village, Michigan Survey Instrument Pre-Test Survey Distribution Analytical Methods iv page viii 10 12 12 13 15 19 22 23 23 24 26 27 31 33 34 36 ANALYSIS Risk Perception Demographic Profile Demographic Issues Hypotheses Testing DISCUSSION Conclusions and Recommendations General Environmental Issues Transportation Risk Factors Site Risk Factors Hypotheses Structure of the Study The Survey Questionnaire Structure APPENDIX A APPENDIX B APPENDIX C APPENDIX D LIST OF REFERENCES page 37 37 42 43 59 61 61 62 63 65 66 67 73 74 75 78 80 LIST OF TABLES page 1. USEPA Survey of Public's Risks .......................................................... 16 2. Socioeconomic Profile of Case Study Areas ........................................ 35 3. Summary of Variables ........................................................................... 38 4. Mean Perception Scores for General Environmental Issues ................. 4O 5. Mean Perception Scores for Transportation Risk Factors .................... 41 6. Mean Perception Scores for Site Risk Factors ........................................ 42 7. Demographic Profile of Survey Respondents ....................................... 44 8. Bivariate Correlation Coefficients of LENGTH, EDUC, PROXIMITY 45 9. Bivariate Correlation Coefficients of ENVIRO, TRANS, and SITE 46 10. Difference of Means T—Test between Dexter Village and Miamisburg 47 11. Difference of Means T-Test between Female and Male Respondents 48 12. Regression of TOTAL and EDUC ........................................................ 49 13. Regression of TOTAL, EDUC, and TOWN ......................................... 50 14. Regression of TOTAL, EDUC, TOWN and ET ................................... 51 15. Regression of TOTAL and PROXIMITY ............................................ 52 16. Regression of TOTAL, PROXIMITY, and TOWN .............................. 53 17. Regression of TOTAL, PROXIMITY, TOWN, and PT ....................... 54 18. Regression of TOTAL and LENGTH .................................................. SS 19. Regression of TOTAL, LENGTH, and TOWN ................................... 56 20. Regression of TOTAL, LENGTH, TOWN, and LT ............................. 58 - page 21. Review of Hypothesis Outcomes ........................................................... 58 22. Sample Of Reconstructed Survey ......................................................... 7O vii LIST OF FIGURES . Case Study Areas Map ............................................................................... . Miamisburg, Ohio ...................................................................................... . Dexter Village, Michigan ........................................................................... . Comparison of TOTAL and PT Slope/Intercept Values .............................. . Comparison of TOTAL and LENGTH Slope/Intercept Values ................... viii page 25 29 3O 54 57 Chapter I: Introduction and Background Concern over human-made environmental hazards has increased within the last few decades (Slovic 1986). Media attention regarding spills, accidents, and chemical catastrophes has initiated heightened environmental attention within the United States. Organizations and pro-active groups have formed to make citizens aware of current Operations and technological advances impacting the environment. Within this larger environmental context, perceptions concerning hazardous material transportation is a tOpic that merits investigation. Hazardous materials are identified as any material with one or more of the following characteristics: ignitability, corrosivity, reactivity, or toxicity. Current estimates of the amount of hazardous materials generated annually are 22 billion pounds (Cunningham 1994). The United States Environmental Protection Agency (U .S.E.P.A.) lists hazardous materials disposal, at both active and abandoned sites, as the first and second most important concerns facing the American public (Roberts 1990). There is recognition of hazardous materials in manufacturing and recognition of the disposal risks, but little research has addressed the intermediary step of transportation of hazardous materials. 2 Therefore, I would like to study the perceptions of two sets of respondents concerning the rail transportation of hazardous materials. The study areas for this thesis are Dexter Village, Michigan, and Miamisburg, Ohio. The study areas were chosen for their location within the industrial belt, the amount of hazardous materials moving within their state, and the towns' proximity to the rail lines. The explicit study of environmental perception, as a theme of behavioral geography and environmental psychology, began in the 19503. Besides focusing on one’s " images of place," the early environmental perception literature concentrated on detailing natural hazard perception (Burton and Kates 1964; White 1960; Calef 1950; Flora 1956). This natural hazard perception concurred with the environmental determinists’ study of environmental influence, or how the environment was thought to influence an individual‘s perceptions and attitudes of the real world. Environmental perception literature reflected upon the subjective images of the inhabitants to the hazardous areas (Arbuthnot 1977; Borden and Francis 1978; Borden and Schettino 1979; Bruvold 1973; Calef 1950; Craik and lube 1976; Dunlap and Van Liere 1978; Goodey 1974; Slovic 1986). Within environmental perception research, the area of perceived risks has received increasing attention. Slovic suggests that studies of risk perception should examine the judgments people make when they are asked to characterize and evaluate hazardous activities and technologies (1986). As geographers have attempted to explain relationships between humans and the surrounding environment, studies of perception have continued to develop. Past arguments have questioned whether one's activity within the environment is determined not only directly by the form and characteristics of the 3 environment, but also indirectly by one's perception of the environment (Goodey 1974). Today, risk perception research is concerned with providing a basis for understanding and for planning how citizens will respond to a hazard and for improving the interaction of technical experts within the field, the average citizen, and those in decision making positions. Research on risk perception has identified measurable factors that influence how individuals perceive environmental issues. If important characteristics that predict perception can be identified, it may be possible to aScertain how safe is safe enough. Some of the individual characteristics that have been linked to environmental perceptions include demographics, distance, and length of residence (Arbuthnot 1977; Borden and Francis 1978; Borden and Schettino 1979; Craik and lube 1976; Dunlap and Van Liere 1978). Past research has been successful at identifying relationships between these characteristics and perception. However, there is disagreement over which characteristics are the best indicators of perception. This thesis will attempt to look at several independent variables and to statistically determine if they are indicative of the perceived risk fi'om hazardous material transportation. Past studies have tried to describe risk perceptions in conjunction with socioeconomic characteristics of the respondents. Questions regarding whether gender and educational status have influenced the level of perception have suggested differing accounts (Johnson and Buttel 1977; Slovic 1991). Geographic measures such as the proximity to an industry or transportation corridor have also been discussed in perception studies (Slovic 1994.) This thesis will examine several factors that may account for 4 variations in the perception of risk fi'om transporting hazardous materials. The factors, previously discussed in the literature, which are incorporated in this study include gender (Arbuthnot 1977), education (Bruvold 1973), length of residence (Lindell and Earle 1994), and proximity to the rail (Slovic 1991). One additional variable analyzed in this study is whether a difference in risk exists between the two case study areas. This comparison is incorporated in this study to ascertain what difl‘erences may exist between an area where a rail incident has occurred and an area unafi‘ected by the transportation of hazardous materials. By examining socioeconomic factors which may influence risk perceptions, a better understanding of the public's risk perceptions may be reached. Past literature has analyzed what factors may be influential in determining risk levels (Bunting and Guelke 1979; Buttel and Johnson 1977); however, this study implements these factors into a comparison of the two study areas. The first factor presented in this study is the role of gender in determining perceived risk. Past risk studies involving gender have suggested that women have a heightened sense of environmental awareness (Schahn and Holzer 1990) resulting in higher perceived risk levels. Other studies simply state that the environmental and Equal Rights movements have coincided; therefore, leaving another avenue for female empowerment (Borden and Francis 1978). This study will attempt to contribute to the volume of risk perception literature by determining if gender is influential in environmental risk perception. Another socioeconomic factor which may contribute to perceived risk is the extent Of one's education. Education has been touted as a key to environmental awareness. 5 Presumably, the higher the level Of one's education the greater the exposure to various ideas (Arbuthnot 1977). Environmental activeness and awareness are thought to indicate higher perceived levels of risk. Geographic assessment is also necessary when trying to analyze risk perceptions. The length of one‘s residence in an area and their proximity to the studied area (for the purpose of this study: a rail line) have shown differing accounts in resident's perceived risk levels. A lower level of perceived risk may be attributed to the greater length of residence and the continued transportation of the materials without any exposure or dispersal problems. Slovic M (1991) and Lindell and Earle (1994) concurred in their studies that those living closer to the rail viewed the transporting of materials as safer. The generation of hazardous materials and the treatment, storage, or disposal of such substances has been the primary focus of past studies, while concern for the risk of transporting the materials has received little attention. The transportation of such materials allows for several factors to contribute to the overall risk. These risk factors include any possible damages to the highway or rail track, weather conditions, or routing concerns which may occur along the transportation route, ultimately resulting in the increased likelihood of a dispersal. With the strengthening of regulations concerning hazardous material disposal, many generators of hazardous materials have found that treating or storing the material onsite has become more burdensome than shipping the material to a treatment or disposal facility. This burden has resulted in an increase in the transportation of materials to Off-site facilities where the treatment process can continue. Rail transportation, the 6 subject of this thesis, has been one means of transportation used to transfer hazardous materials from the production plants to the final treatment facilities. One reason for utilizing rail for transporting hazardous materials is the fact that it is more than four times safer than highway transportation. In terms of price, rail is cheaper than truck transport due to the pricing scheme based on volume rather than distance hauled. Although motor trucks are the primary mode of transporting hazardous materials, rail is used with great frequency and safety- 1,497,464 origins in 1993, with 99.99% reaching their destinations safely (American Railroad Association Annual Report 1993). While many governmental entities have focused on highway transportation in their routing restrictions in urban areas, few routing precautions have been incorporated in rail legislation According to the American Railroad Association (ARA), there are two types Of train mishaps: accident or incident. An accident is a situation in which a rail car of hazardous materials derails, catches fire, explodes, suffers a serious sideswipe, or involves death. It does not have to release its contents (but may), meet any dollar damage figure, or any other requirement. An incident is the unintentional release of a hazardous material while in transportation (including loading and unloading). This release does not usually involve an accident (ARA Annual Report 1993). The focus Of this study is how citizens viewed the risks of transporting hazardous materials by rail and what, if any, socioeconomic aspects may contribute to the degree of risk. Several risk factors for transporting hazardous materials have been identified. The type of load, weather, rail condition, and time of day are potential contributors to any type of rail mishap (Schierbl 1993; Saccomanno and Shortreed 1994). One problem addressed 7 by the ARA is that of material container types. While hazardous materials are transported by rail in various types of rail cars, such as tank cars, covered hoppers, gondolas, box cars, trailers, and/or containers on flat cars, the majority (70%) of hazardous materials move in tank cars. In 1993, the number of train accidents in which a hazardous material car was damaged or derailed was 262 (ARA Annual Report 1993). Of the non-accident releases, 89% were from tank cars. Hazardous materials have long been transported by rail, yet little attention has been given to the risk perceived by adjacent residents. As the role of transporting hazardous materials by rail will only increase due to the continued production of such substances, the need for proper assessment Of those risks perceived by the adjacent public is vital. Due to the high volume of hazardous material generation and transportation within the industrial belt of the United States, I have chosen to address risk perceptions in two towns within this belt. My working hypotheses are: 1. The residents of Miamisburg, Ohio, will indicate a higher total degree Of risk perception concerning transportable hazardous materials than their Dexter Village, Michigan, counterparts. The higher perception of risk is expected due to Miamisburg's past experience with a derailment, evacuation, and chemical dispersal in 1986. Another factor contributing to Miamisburg's heightened awareness is its continued exposure to the DOE Mound facility, 8 which is currently undergoing corrective action. By first addressing comparisons between the two communities I can further delve into variables which may be indicators of possible reasons for certain perceptions. 2. Female respondents will indicate higher levels of perceived risk than the male respondents. The difl‘erence in gender perception is expected because of the need for women to assert leadership within a societal movement or because of heightened environmental concern. The focus in the existing literature has concerned the difference in how the sexes perceive risk and why females are likely to indicate higher levels of risk. This study will focus on the differences between the combined females and males in the two study areas. 3. Education is a significant indicator of perceived risk level, and those with higher levels will perceive greater risk than those with less formal education. The extent of one‘s education may relate to awareness of environmental issues and current technologies in existence. Past literature has suggested a relationship between education and perceived risk levels of environmental issues (Arbuthnot 1977). This study attempts to test this education relationship in a comparative analysis of Dexter Village and Miamisburg respondents' risk levels. 9 4. Proximity to the rail line is a significant indicator of risk and respondents living closest will indicate higher risk levels than those at a distance. Prior to addressing the relationship between the proximity of the respondent and the rail line to the level of perceived risk, one must look at what distance defines a risk exposure area. The establishment of such an area should be completed prior to the distribution of a survey instrument. Slovic surveyed respondents in an exposure area of two miles fiom the Oregon highway where hazardous materials were being transported (1991). Within this two mile area Slovic found varying attitudes toward the transportation Of hazardous materials. High risk perception levels were found for respondents living nearer the transportation corridor within the two mile survey area. The two mile area defined the distance at which a highway accident would disrupt residents, but the closer their proximity to the rail line the higher their perception of risk. A two mile exposure area was chosen as an adequate distance for the level of toxicity of the materials being transported. When studying risk exposure areas, the extent of the area is based upon the type and volume of chemicals being transported. Like Slovic, this thesis looked at risk perceptions of residents up to two miles from the rail lines. Within both study areas, this two mile zone encompasses the greatest concentration of the residents. Beyond this distance, the population density decreases. By defining my survey area I could assess the attitudes of residents at varying distances from the rail lines. A greater concern is expected to exist for those adjacent to the rail rather than those further away, because of the constant threat of not only a 10 dispersal but also a derailment or other accident (Flynn eLal 1990). Due to Miamisburg's derailment history it is expected that the respondents closer to the rail were more affected than those firrther away ultimately resulting in a greater risk perception. This study will focus on the proximity of the respondents and analyze whether a respondent's distance affects risk perceptions and whether the effects of distance are stronger in Miamisburg than in Dexter Village. 5. The length of residence is a significant indicator of risk levels and respondents with greater lengths of residence will indicate higher levels Of risk than those who have resided in the areas shorter amounts of time. Miamisburg respondents who have lived in the area prior to the 1986 derailment are likely to perceive higher levels of risk due to the derailment, evacuation, and chemical exposure. Miamisburg's familiarity with the extensive problems of the DOE Mound facility can contribute to their overall awareness. Structure of thiiILapg The general nature of the problem(s) and the risks associated with the transportation of hazardous materials by rail, as well as the study's objectives and hypotheses are presented in Chapter 1. Chapter II includes a detailed review of risk perception literature pertaining to the hazardous material process and transportation, variables which may influence the perceptions, and laws and regulations developed to minimize the risks. 11 The study's methods, fi'om questionnaire development and distribution to study area demographics are presented in Chapter III. Chapter IV presents the survey results and statistical analysis in graphic, tabular, and map form. A comprehensive discussion Of the study's results and their relationship to the primary hypotheses of this study is presented in Chapter V. Chapter II: Relevant Literature A discussion of the socioeconomic factors, their relation to risk perception, and how they are integrated into this study will be presented in this chapter. The New Environmental Paradigm Considerable attention has been given to the emerging environmental consciousness within the American society since the early 1970‘s. The advent Of organizations, regulations, and grassroots activism has contributed to this overall interest in natural resources. In a 197 8 article, Dunlap and Van Liere suggested that a “New Environmental Paradigm“ (NEP) was emerging in society that confronted the older view of an anthropocentric, anti-ecological order. By constructing a 12-item NEP scale, with Likert-formatted questions, the opposing views of how humans regard the natural environment was tested. After subjecting the data to analysis, Dunlap and Van Liere concluded that the 12 items which comprised the NEP scale formed "an internally consistent and unidirnensional scale (1978)." Dunlap and Van Liere's analysis is a prototype for risk perception studies such as this comparative assessment of two towns concerning hazardous materials transportation. The NEP scale is necessary in developing an instrument by which to assess perceptions. Dunlap and Van Liere’s new environmental 12 13 paradigm describes this emerging concern for one's own health and that of the natural environment. The NEP is integral to perception studies as it reflects the growing trend toward environmental sensitivity. Risk E erts vs. La le Risk assessment is a decision making tool in environmental planning. Risk assessment is viewed as one part of risk analysis by the Council on Environmental Quality (Cohrssen and Covello 1989). It is the second step in a process which includes identifying the hazard, assessing the risk, determining the significance of the risk, and communicating the risk. The assessment phase estimates "the severity and likelihood of harm to human health or the environment“ (Cohrssen and Covello 1989). It is often used in controversial planning situations as the basis for regulatory decisions including standard setting and permit approval. Risk assessment can heighten conflict in some planning situations because it provides numbers to debate. The result of risk assessment is a probability of harm from exposure to the materials and processes. The probability is used to identify management strategies to reduce the risk to de minimus levels. De minimus non carat [ex is a legal doctrine that means the "law does not care for, or take notice of, very small or trifling matters" (Black 1990). De rninimus risk is referred to as a risk of one in a million during a 70 year lifetime. Risks are subjective though, as some people view an estimate of deaths from cancer (ex. 1 in 1,000,000) as too much, while others dismiss the risk. The disagreement is not over the estimated numbers, but in how peOple perceive the numbers. Even when 14 the source and accuracy of risk assessment data are agreed on, there is Often conflict concerning the differences in perception of the data. While the subjectiveness of risks are debated, a discrepancy also exists between these experts active within the environmental field and the average citizen with little technical knowledge. Concerning the perceived risks associated with hazardous materials, the technologically-competent analysts are inclined to rely on risk assessments, while the vast number of citizens use intuitive risk judgments or risk perceptions. One of the best examples of this contrast is evident in the United States Environmental Protection Agency's (U SEPA) survey comparing the American public's environmental concerns with those of the USEPA The American public listed active and abandoned hazardous waste sites as the numbers one and two highest concerns (Roberts 1990). See Table 1. Of the top ten environmental risks, an additional four were concerned with the exposure to various chemicals as a result of accidents or releases from industrial sites. The heightened concern for hazardous material handling demonstrated in the USEPA survey provides the basis for surveys such as mine. The insecurities exhibited by the USEPA's public survey respondents concerning hazardous materials on contained sites is the foundation for examining the attitudes toward the same materials in a moving environment. In contrast to the public’s risk perceptions, the USEPA's highest concerns did not mention hazardous waste sites. Sandman, egg (1993), suggest that the public-versus-expert risk controversies are due to the public's lack of understanding concerning scientific data. This " science illiteracy" is addressed by using venues like the mass media to distribute technical 15 information. Starr (1985) suggests that the "acceptance of any risk is more dependent on the public’s confidence in risk management than on the quantitative estimates of risk. " This thesis looks at the perception of risks of the common citizen. By choosing to study two areas, I was able to examine the perceptions of two sets of citizens who have had varying degrees of interaction with hazardous materials. The extent of this variance will be examined in Chapter IV. Factors Influencing Risk Permtion Risk perception research is concerned with providing a basis for understanding and planning how citizens will respond to various scenarios. An integral part of assessing risk is determining if any background factors contribute to the level of perceived risk. Several suggestions have been given concerning what demographic and socioeconomic factors may contribute to environmental interest. Four variables (education, gender, length of residence, and proximity to the rail) selected from the previous literature concerning risk perception and introduced in Chapter I, will be incorporated into the analysis of the responses from Dexter Village and Miamisburg for further comparison of risk perceptions between the two study areas. Each variable will be discussed in this chapter and then analyzed within Chapter IV. 16 Table l: USEPA Survey of the Public's Perception of Environmental Risks lg'sk Percent of Respondents who Indicated VERY SERIOUS Active Hazardous Waste Sites 62 Abandoned Hazardous Waste Sites 61 Worker Exposure to Toxic Substances 60 Industrial Water Pollution 58 Nuclear Accident Radiation 58 Radioactive Waste 55 Underground Tank Leaks 54 Pesticide Harm to Users 54 Pesticide Residues 52 Industrial Accident Pollution 51 Farm Run-off Water Pollution 50 Tap Water Contamination 49 Industrial Air Pollution 48 Ozone Layer Destruction 47 Sewage Plant Water Pollution 45 Vehicle Exhaust 38 Tanker, Rig Oil Spills 38 Acid Rain 36 Urban Run-Ofl' Water Pollution 35 Damaged Wetlands 35 Genetic Alteration Damage 35 Nonhazardous Waste Sites 33 Greenhouse Effect 33 Indoor Air Pollution 26 X-Ray Radiation 22 Source: Richard Morgenstem and Stuart Sessions, "Weighing Environmental Risks: EPA'S Unfinished Business" Environment 30 (1988): 14-39. l7 Constantini and Hanf submit that an individual's degree of concern may be closely associated with more firndamental social and political attitudes as well as general social role and background (1972). One of the dominant characterizations is that environmental consciousness has been largely a middle and upper class phenomenon, which is measured in terms of education, income, and occupation (McEvoy 1971; Hany M 1969). Harry gt_al discussed the attitudes of those involved in conservation associations in the Pacific Northwest and found that membership in the movement appears to be composed largely of upper-middle-class citizens (1969). Environmental concern may also be attributed to education and past experience. In their study of Lake Tahoe residents, Constantini and Hanf described those citizens who were better educated as displaying a higher level of environmental concern (1972). Arbuthnot concurred with this sentiment that, in the Athens, Ohio, area, those persons with higher educations practiced a higher degree of recycling and had more environmental concerns in the Athens, Ohio area (197 7). Presumably, those citizens with greater levels have been exposed to more ideas and are more apt to be involved within the community. This study analyzes the educational levels of the respondents and statistically compares the relationship to perceived risk in the two study areas. Gender is an additional factor that may contribute to how one perceives a risk. Past studies have suggested differing accounts, as positive relations in one study (Van Liere and Dunlap 1981) are reversed in another (Arcury, Scollay, and Johnson 1987). Increased environmental concern by women has been reported as higher values in attitude scales and measures of self-reported behavior, but less knowledge about environmental 18 problems has been displayed by women (Schahn and Holzer 1990). Borden and Francis suggest that the increased environmental concern by women may be attributed to a potential for leadership, as the environmental movement has evolved with the women's movement (197 8). Slovic etg discuss that female respondents indicated the transportation of hazardous materials as less safe than did their male counterparts (1991). Slovic 1a; described in their study of transporting hazardous materials by highway the demographic aspects of the respondents along the corridor (1991); those respondents living closer to the corridor saw transportation as safer. Lindell and Earle concurred that the perception Of risk from hazardous facilities is often lower for those living closest to the facilities (1994). Slovic eLa_l_ analyzed the responses of Oregon residents up to two miles from the highway. The two mile distance fi'om the highway is considered the risk exposure area or the zone in which an accident involving the transported materials could put the adjacent community at risk for evacuation or potential health problems. Slovic's study looked at a relatively small-scale exposure area due to the volume and type of materials being transported. Other risk exposure areas can extend several hundred miles from the site of the accident. Recent events resulting in enormous higher order impacts include the chemical manufacturing accident at Bhopal, India, and the meltdown of the nuclear reactor at Chemoybl. Following these extreme events are a myriad of mishaps varying in the breadth and size of their impacts, ultimately affecting larger populations within greater exposure areas (Saccomanno and Shortreed 1993). Planners have attempted to decide at what distance does a risk dissipate (Saccomanno and Shortreed 1993). The need to define a consistent distance is unnecessary as each dispersal is site-specific. Past l9 literature has looked at small-scale case risk exposure areas such as those analyzed in this study (Slovic 1991). The determination of a risk exposure area is dependent upon the population density, type of material, and volume of the material. The density of the two study areas combined with the proximity of the town populations to rail lines provides for a representative sample to exist within the defined two mile area. One's length of residence in the community can contribute to their perceived risk level. Those who have been subjected to continual exposure without any difficulty may be more likely to indicate a lower level of risk. A lower level of risk may be attributed to the greater length Of residence due to the continued transportation of the materials without any exposure or dispersal problems. As hazardous material usage continues to increase, the issue of treatment of the material has become an increasingly complicated issue. As the difiiculty in treating materials and waste on-site has escalated, the importance of reliable transportation to such commercial outlets has been growing. This reliance upon transportation, especially rail, as one component of the hazardous material cycle is the primary reason for further exploring the public’s attitudes and reactions to the corresponding risks involved. P i n n 'n Tr in H ri Due to the increase of regulations regarding hazardous materials, many producers of hazardous wastes and materials have resorted to Off-site, licensed disposal facilities and storage units. This increase in off-site treatment has resulted in increased transportation of the hazardous materials. Analysis by the United States Department of Transportation 20 indicates that hazardous material shipments impose significant risks to the population. Between 1971 and 1980, more than 111,000 accidents involving hazardous materials were reported, resulting in a total of 248 fatalities, 6873 injuries, and approximately $120 million in property damage. Railroad accidents accounted for 8% Of the accidents and 18% of fatalities (National Transportation Safety Board 1981). Few researchers have described the risk perception of transporting any type of hazardous material. One of the first studies was done by Slovic et_a_l in Oregon. It was an initial description of a program to transport radioactive waste fi'om the Hanford Reserve in Washington through Oregon by highway (1991). Of the Oregon residents surveyed, 69.1% agreed that present methods for transporting hazardous materials out of the Reserve are necessary for proper remediation activities to occur. Regarding current methods for transporting hazardous materials, half of the respondents (50.7%) concluded that today’s techniques are reasonably safe; however, respondents viewed transportation as a riskier process than storage. Flynn :13! studied the perceptions of risk and benefits associated with a nuclear waste repository, trust in the Department of Energy, and views on other issues pertaining to nuclear waste (1990). Their study indicated that people perceived the transportation Of nuclear waste as highly risky. The percentage of respondents who agreed or strongly agreed to “Highway and rail accidents will occur in transporting the wastes to the repository site, " was 77.4% in Nevada, 69.2% in California, and 71.6% nationally (1990). The process of transporting hazardous materials is integral in the reduction of the volume of materials being stored on site in a particular region or community. This 21 reduction in the volume of materials stored on site may be a benefit to the community shipping the materials, yet it may instantly become a nuisance for the receiving community. The answer to this dilemma is found in acceptable, safe procedures used to permanently deter the materials from infringing upon the natural environment or the surrounding communities. Safe procedures are vital, but the reality of transporting hazardous materials is evident within the two study areas. The focus of this study is on the perceived risk of the transportation of such materials and what factors may influence the degree of risk perceived. Beyond assessing environmental risks, Oregon respondents concurred with many of the images offered by those who are reactive to residential facility sitings. Respondents believed that areas through which radioactive wastes are transported are likely to be unattractive to business development and tourism. Respondents also indicated little confidence in the state of Oregon to provide accurate information concerning the transportation process. Within the survey, 88% of the images evoked by word association were negative (Slovic 1991). The transporting of hazardous materials results in several issues which can benefit from perception studies. The ”unattractiveness" of areas where routing may occur can lead to undevelopment and lower land values. The lack of information flow fi'om those in authoritative positions can Often result in little confidence in ofiicials dealing with the transportation process. The risks to the health of the environment and the citizens who are subjected to the materials through routing plans can be better assessed. 22 Regulations Most of the environmental regulations concerning the transportation of hazardous materials have been formulated in response to the public's perception of various levels of risk. Federal regulations (49 CFR Part 397) have established standards for maintaining and enforcing designated highway routes for truck shipments of hazardous materials. Ohio's regulations were reactionary due to the derailment of the train in Miamisburg. Ohio House Bill 428 was signed on June 24, 1988, in Miamisburg by then governor Richard F. Celeste in response to the derailment which had occurred two years earlier. With the implementation of Ohio's Hazardous Material Transportation Act (OHMTA), a registration program for all haulers was created along with a fee collection and disbursement program for emergency response training. The OHMTA also instituted a new system of enforcement by imposing civil forfeitures rather than criminal penalties. In relation to rail transportation, rules in Ohio and Michigan are consistent with the Federal regulations. Both states invoke regulations similar to 49 CFR. The relevance of Ohio House Bill 428 to my study is important, as the very people who were affected by the derailment are those same individuals questioned by this survey. The events which happened in Miamisburg brought awareness of the risks of transporting materials through communities. Prior to this event, few regulations were in place. The effectiveness of these regulations in difi’using any potential risks to the citizens may be established by analyzing their perceptions of the continuing transportation of such materials. As mentioned previously, perception of risk is a vital prerequisite of the planning process and creation of legislation. Chapter 11]: Methods This chapter discusses the procedures I used to numerically rank and geographically analyze the risks associated with general environmental issues and transportation risk factors. Divided into four parts, this chapter focuses on research design, including the Objectives for the study. Sections two and three of the chapter concern the study area and the survey instrument respectively. The final section includes the techniques for questionnaire distribution and analysis of the respondent demographics. Research Desigg This analysis was designed to contribute to the understanding of risk perception and to describe the attitudes of residents concerning the transportation of hazardous materials. Utilizing proximity, socioeconomic variables, and a case study approach, this study answers the question of how the two study areas compare and contrast in their views on transportable hazardous materials by rail. The proximity analysis is useful in establishing relationships that may exist between location and degree of perceived risk. In addressing the risk perceptions of hazardous material transportation, I will address how independent variables of gender, education, town, and length of residence contribute to awareness (Borden and Francis 197 8; Schahn and Holzer 1990). 23 24 This research is consistent with Slovic‘s body of research, in that it attempts to measure the perceptions of risk in transporting hazardous materials and subject the results to statistical analysis. Slovic has discussed the use of the "psychometric paradigm" which has explored the usefirlness of psychophysical scaling methods combined with those of multivariate analysis in producing representations Of risk attitudes and perceptions (1986). Past researchers have used the psychometric paradigm in asking people to judge the riskiness or safety of various hazardous activities, materials, and technologies and to indicate their desires for risk reduction and regulation of these hazards. These judgments have then been related to decisions about the hazards position on various qualitative characteristics of risk. Study Area To better explain attitudes toward the transportation of hazardous materials by rail, I have analyzed and compared the responses of residents in two different areas, one of which has experienced an accident and one of which has not. The two areas, Dexter Village, Michigan and Miamisburg, Ohio were chosen for the comparable socioeconomic composition of their populations, railroad proximity to the population, population size, and the fact that hazardous materials are being transported on the railroads through these areas. Michigan and Ohio's location within the industrial belt of the United States and their past records of train incidents were also considered. 25 Figure 1: Dexter Village, Michigan and Miamisburg, Ohio CASE STUDY AREAS DEXTER VILLAGE, MICHIGAN MIAMISBURG, OHIO 26 Miamisburg, Ohio Miamisburg, Ohio, situated in the southwest corner of the state, is located 10 miles southwest of Dayton. The United States Census lists the population of Miamisburg as 17,834 (1990). The railroad tracks are situated between houses through the center of the city. The income and educational levels of Miamisburg residents are diverse while the racial composition is homogeneous (98% Caucasian). Miamisburg was also the scene of the largest evacuation in the United States due to a train accident and chemical dispersal. On July 8, 1986, a southbound Baltimore and Ohio train traveling from Walbridge, Ohio, to Cincinnati derailed in Mianrisburg. The train was carrying thirty-five cars, several of which were chemical containers. While traveling across the Great Miami River, at a speed of 45 mph, the train hit an area of track which had expanded due to repair work and the heat, causing the 24th thru the 38th cars to derail. The 30th car, a load of white phosphorous, ignited and burned, resulting in the evacuation of a two mile area adjacent to the rail line (PUCO Report 1986). Area fire departments arrived immediately and began to remediate the bunting phosphorous by applying water. This process continued for 24 hours (into July 9th) at which time the fire stopped and water spraying ended. At 6:05pm on July 9th, the phosphorous car reignited, and exploded, resulting in a second evacuation. According to Bill Mitchell, a Federal Railroad Inspector at the scene, the phosphorous tank was sprayed with water again until it was finally decided on Friday July 11th to vent the car and install fans for a hot and fast burn. By 7:00 am, Saturday July 12th, the fire was finally extinguished (Bill Mitchell, personal interview, July 1995). 27 Miamisburg is an ideal location for assessing how people perceive hazardous material transportation risks, as the 1986 deraihnent was the largest of its kind. The evacuation of over 15,000 residents made startlingly evident the idea of hazardous material transportation risks. Miamisburg is not only a favorable area due to the derailment, but situated at the southern tip of the city is the United States Department Of Energy Mound Facility. DOE Mound manufactures non-nuclear and tritium-containing weapon components. In their production processes, Sanitary, hazardous, radioactive, and mixed wastes are generated. Environmental restoration currently includes a clean up program with decontamination and decommissioning activities. Miamisburg residents' interaction with hazardous materials is a daily issue and it is for this reason that I wanted to try to better assess their perceptions Of risk. The initial derailment of the phosphorous tanker resulted in the evacuation of the respondents within a half mile of the spill. The risk exposure area of the Miamisburg derailment was broadened due to the actions of the emergency response crews. The phosphorous fire that resulted fi'om the derailment was doused with water causing a mushroom cloud to form and move down through the Miami Valley. This resulted in a larger exposure area and a total of 15,000 people being evacuated. Dexter Village, Michigan Dexter Village, Michigan, situated in the southern lower peninsula, is located approximately 11 miles northwest of Ann Arbor. The village population is listed as 1,497 with most residents residing within one mile of the railroad tracks. Income of the 28 residents of Dexter Village are highly varied. The residents are racially homogeneous (Caucasian), but vary in educational levels and occupations (1990 United States Census). Dexter Village does not have a history of rail accidents, but the line that runs through the town is actively transporting hazardous materials. Miamisburg and Dexter Village are small towns, yet physically adjacent to small cities (Dayton and Ann Arbor). This proximity facilitates emergency planning, by the LEPC (Local Emergency Planning Committee) and it provides a larger body of contingency workers capable of handling emergencies involving hazardous materials. One interest of mine was to see if information about the transporting of hazardous materials had been dispersed to the residents adjacent to the route. A respondent's higher perceived level of risk could be attributed to this information flow. This research was designed to analyze geographically the perceptions of risk in transporting hazardous materials through two study areas by rail. By partitioning regions within the study areas to distribute questionnaires and trace responses, I could subject all responses to proximity measurements. For each of the study areas, I analyzed aerial photographs of the regions and then partitioned the 20 residential blocks closest to the rail line as my study regions. 29 Figure 2: Miamisburg, Ohio Scale: 1224,000 Source: United States Geological Survey. State of Ohio, Miamisburg Quadrangle. 1987. (Enlarged 10%) 30 Figure 3: Dexter Village, Michigan 53"., V "- 7," ‘ \ .. 37 “(a 3:. "All ’3 ' o 1. _~-\ \ - ,/' Y k‘ I ' ..., fl, -\ \. , var/"x \u 4.3: y t l . I I ,, a; . - ’ L2“ . , ' 'm , . . ' “L3 ‘ I l ' 1" DAN awas- Scale: 1:24,000 Source: United States Geological Survey. State of Michigan, Dexter Village Quadrangle. 1987. (Enlarged 10%) 31 urv Instrument Risk perception is the threat that an individual believes is associated with a specific environmental issue. The measurement of risk perception in this study is accomplished by assessing opinions. Three categories of risk perception are used: the perception of environmental risks in general, the perception of risks associated with the transportation of hazardous materials, and risks associated with specific factors in Dexter Village and Miamisburg (such as the Huron and Great Miami Rivers). The survey instrument utilized to assess the risk perception was a questionnaire with closed-ended questions (see Appendix C). The questionnaire was divided into three sections corresponding to the three categories above. A fourth section detailing respondent demographics was also included. Sections one through three allowed the respondent to indicate their risk perception level on a Likert style rating scale of 0 (low risk) to 6 (high risk) (de Vaus 1990). Section four allowed for both fill-in style questions, scale, and yes or no responses. The questionnaire, which was gee—coded and stratified by zone begins with general questions concerning human health and the environment and then progressively becomes more site-specific. The first section of questions required the respondents to indicate the level of risk they associated with various environmental pollutants. The environmental issues for the general perception measure came from a comparative risk assessment conducted by the United States Environmental Protection Agency. In that study, EPA scientists defined and ranked 31 environmental issues on the basis of human health, ecological, and welfare risks (Morgenstem and Session 1988). For this research only 8 of those issues were included in the questionnaire, including the threat of water contamination and the possible exposure of 32 the rivers within the two towns. Worker exposure is an inherent risk factor in any type Of hazardous material transportation as is a decrease in air quality should a dispersal occur. The second section of the questionnaire looks at factors of transporting hazardous materials and the extent to which they are seen as a risk to the environment. The transportation risk factors used in this study were delineated by the Oflice of Technology Assessment in their report on the Transportation of Hazardous Materials (1988). These factors are concerned with the (1) condition of the rail, (2) weather factors, (3) type of load, and (4) amount of material being transported. Saccomanno and Shortreed acknowledge these factors in their study of the perception of societal and individual risks associated with transportable hazardous materials (1994). Again, respondents indicated their perceived risk on a Likert response scale from 0 (NO Risk) to 6 (High Risk). The third section presents environmental features which may be at risk within the study areas. Both study areas have rivers adjacent to the towns. Other potential risks included those associated with the proximity to the tracks, the vulnerability of the towns, and the respondent's home. The final section of the questionnaire asked the respondent to indicate various socioeconomic attributes. The demographic factors were included for further analysis of any correlations between the five independent variables (gender, education, income, proximity, and length of residence) and level of risk perceived. Following the demographic factors were either three (for Dexter Village) or four (for Miamisburg) additional site specific questions. The survey for Dexter Village included an inquiry into whether the respondent was aware of any transportation of hazardous materials through 33 the village. The awareness of the residents was questioned due to little acknowledgment by the rail and county oflicials of rail transportation of such materials in Dexter Village. The main focus of the Washtenaw County officials has been on the highway routing Of such materials. The July 1986 derailment and evacuation in Miamisburg made such a question unnecessary there. Other questions asked if the respondent was involved in any type of environmental Organizations and if they felt that their elected Oficials were addressing environmental issues. The survey instrument allows the respondents to indicate their perceptions of risk for general environmental issues as well as factors concerned specifically with the transportation of hazardous materials. Since each issue had a scale fi'om 0 to 6, three intermediary scores (environmental issues, transportation issues, and site issues) and a final score for the total level of risk can be tallied for each respondent questionnaire. lira-lest According to Dillman's (1978) outline for pre-testing, the questionnaire was administered to three sets of people: professionals within the field, others associated with the field, and others outside of the field who represent a cross section of the population to be studied. For the purpose of this study, I distributed 10 pre-test questionnaires. Four respondents were employees of the Ohio Environmental Protection Agency, Division of Hazardous Waste Management. Three test respondents were fiom the Ohio Environmental Protection Agency employed in positions that are not directly related to hazardous waste management but support divisions such as Data and Systems and 34 Groundwater. The remaining three respondents came fiom outside of the environmental field and were representative of the intended study respondents. The feedback from the pro-test allowed for changes to the questionnaire prior to the distribution of the main survey. The comments fi'om the pre-test group indicated a need for clarity of the questions (re-wording) and a need for examples of how to read and answer each statement. Once these changes were made, I resubmitted the questionnaire to the pilot group for feedback. The clarifications addressed the pilot groups' concerns. Survey Distribution The questionnaire was distributed by a stratified scheme (de Vaus 1990) in both Dexter Village and Miamisburg. This stratification process was developed fiom aerial photographs of each area. A breakdown by city block was seen as the most optimal way to gage proximity to the railroad and the easiest for recording responses. The Dexter Village and Miamisburg study areas included the twenty blocks (zones) closest to the tracks. As mentioned in the previous chapters, the surveys were distributed at homes up to two miles from the rail lines. This distance was chosen as the risk exposure area due to the population density and layout of each town relative to the rail line. The two mile radius also encompassed the evacuation area in Miamisburg from the 1986 derailment. Beyond the two mile zone, the population density decreases. The 100 Dexter questionnaires were distributed on July 29, 1995 and the 100 Miamisburg surveys were delivered on August 19, 1995. Both questionnaires were 35 delivered by hand with stamped envelopes in the survey packet for responses to be returned by mail (Dillrnan 1978). In a follow-up to the initial questionnaire distribution, I targeted those zones from which I had not received sufficient initial survey responses and I returned to Dexter Village on September 10, 1995, and distributed 25 additional surveys. On September 21, 1995, I distributed 15 additional surveys in Miamisburg. In total 53 surveys (22%) Of the 240 distributed were returned completed. Table 2: Socioeconomic Profile of Dexter Village and Miamisburg Dexter Village, MI Miamisburg, OH Population 1,497 17,834 Number of Households 676 6671 Median Household Income $32,411 $32,436 Racial Component 98% Caucasian 98% Caucasian Educational Attainment ‘ (Person 25 yrs and older) 1,019 11,557 High School Grad. 293 (29%) 4317 (37%) Some College 250 (25%) 2086 (18%) Associate's Degree 48 (5%) 769 (7%) Bachelor's Degree 156 (15%) 1246 (11%) Graduate Degree 88 (9%) 336 (3%) Source: 1990 United States Census 36 Analytical Methods After receiving the responses back, I began to sort through the data and begin my analysis. By dividing the questionnaire into three sections, I could use the Environmental, Transportation, and Site issue scores individually or combined. The combined risk levels were calculated by adding the first three Sections of the questionnaire together for each respondent. I then completed preliminary analysis including the means and standard deviations of the issues to ascertain what similarities and differences existed between the two areas. The calculated means of the issues were usefirl in establishing a ranking of the risks. Once the introductory analysis was completed, I could then focus on the testing of my hypotheses. My hypotheses were based on relationships between socioeconomic variables and the perceived risk levels. The methods of statistical analysis I used were t-tests, correlations, and regression analysis to establish what relationships did exist (Slovic 1986). Chapter IV: Analysis In this chapter, the data gathered fi'om the survey instrument outlined in Chapter III are analyzed and the survey results are presented. I will examine the general hypotheses that are the basis for the research, analyze the variables introduced in Chapter I, and discuss the relationships among the variables. The survey measured several variables including risk perception, gender, education, and proximity to the rail. Table 2 summarizes these variables and defines the categories associated with each. The risk perception values were measured on scales, while the socioeconomic values were categorized on the survey instrument and indicated by the respondent. The following discussion provides greater detail about each of the variables. Risk Perception The respondents identified a level of risk represented by a total of 31 issues on a 7-point scale, with 0 being "NO Risk" and 6 being "High Risk." Tables 4 and 5 compare the mean perception scores for each of the general and transportation issues presented in the survey. Table 4 shows the perception of human health and environmental risk fiom the general environmental issues, while Table 5 shows the perceived risk for the transportation issues. Table 6 represents the mean perception scores for the site risk 37 38 factors. The general environmental risks have been identified by the Environmental Protection Agency (EPA) as those issues dominating their agenda at all levels of government. Table 3: Summary of Variables Used in Analysis ENVIRO: Total General Environmental Issues Score; Sum of the sixteen questions that measure respondents perceived level Of risk to human health and environment. TRANS: Total Transportation Risk Factors Score; Sum of the ten questions that measure respondents perceived level of risk. SITE: Total Site-Specific Issues Score; Sum of the five questions that measure respondents perceived level of risk. TOTAL: Total Risk Perception Score, Sum of the ENVIRO, TRANS, and SITE scores. SEX: Gender of Respondent; Indicated by the respondent on the survey instrument. Dummy Variable l = Female, 0 = Male LENGTH: Length of Residence; Indicated by the respondent on the survey instrument; categorized into 7 groups: Less than 1 year, 1 to 2 years, 2 to 5 years, 5 to 10 years, 10 to 15 years, 15 to 25 years, and Greater than 25 years. EDUC: Highest Education Level Attained, Indicated by the respondent on the survey; categorized into 6 groups: Some High School, High School or Equivalent, Some College, College Completed, Technical School, or Graduate School. 39 Table 3 (cont'd) REGION: Geo-coded Residential Blocks determined prior to the Survey Distribution. TOWN: Study Area Designation, Dummy Variable, l = Miamisburg, 0 = Dexter. PROXIMITY: Distance from the railroad, estimated in quarter-mile increments from the center of the designated zone to the rail line. ET: The EDUC level of the respondent multiplied by the respondent's TOWN. PT: The PROXIMITY variable multiplied by the respondent's TOWN. LT: The LENGTH variable multiplied by the respondent's TOWN The mean perception scores were calculated for each section prior to testing the hypothesis. Of the general environmental issues, Dexter Village and Miamisburg respondents indicated 3A "Industrial Discharges to the Surface Water" as the highest perceived level of risk to human health with the mean of 4.774 (sd =1.310). "Industrial Discharges to the surface water" was the risk to the environment that had the highest mean perceived risk level, 4.925 (sd= 1.378). 40 Table 4: Mean Perception Scores for the General Environmental Issues (and Standard Deviation) Perceived Perceived Risk to Risk to Risk Factor Rank Human Health Rank Environment Air Pollutants 3 4.472 (1.370) 3 4.585 (1.262) Toxic Air Pollutants 2 4.755 (1.385) 2 4.604 (1.378) Industrial Discharges 1 4.774 (1.310) 1 4.925 (1.378) Hazardous Waste 5 4.038 (1.480) 5 4.377 (1.484) Accidental Releases 4 4.302 (1.381) 4 4.566 (1.487) Radiation Exposure 7 3.811 (1.582) 6 3.736 (1.711) Drinking Water 8 3.245 (1.440) 8 3.00 (1.732) Worker Exposures 6 3.906 (1.510) 7 3.566 (1.575) The second section of the questionnaire concerned the familiarity of the respondents to issues concerning the transportation of hazardous materials. Within hazardous materials management, there are several factors, identified in Chapter H that can be threats to the environment. Risk factors were again rated on the Likert scale of 0 (Low Risk) to 6 (High Risk). The respondents rated number 7 "Condition of the rail and tank cars" as the factor associated with the highest level of risk (4.811, sd=1.302.) I expected the "condition of the rail" and "population density" to be the highest rated due to Miamisburg's past derailment. 41 Table 5: Mean Perception Scores for the Transportation Risk Factors (and Standard Deviations) Risk Factor Rank Perceived Risk Level Type of Materials 8 4.094 (1.522) Volume of Materials 3 4.321 (1.451) Emergency Response Capability 2 4.340 (1.518) Travel Distance 4 4.302 (1.612) Population Density 5 4.283 (1.486) Natural Resource Density 7 4.245 (1.568) Condition of the Rail 1 4.811 (1.302) Time of Day 10 3.981 (1.704) Weather Conditions 9 4.019 (1.647) Type of Storage 6 4.264 (1.534) The third section of the survey instrument, with risk factors that are site specific to each town surveyed, yielded a highest rating for "the respondent's home," while the "town population” was considered at the least risk in the process of transporting hazardous materials. I expected the "tank car derailment/accident" and "Dexter Village/Miamisburg populations" would be perceived to be at the greatest risk. I submit that the past derailment and the respondent's comments contributed to the above ranking. Until now, I have focused on perception of risk fi'om individual issues. The specific issues (ENVIRO, TRANS, and SITE) are useful in looking at specific risk perception. However, it is interesting to examine the distribution of the total risk perception score of all the issues. The total score is the sum of the risk perception 42 responses to all 31 issues and constitutes the individual's .overall perception of the issues. The mean total risk perception score is 128.1 and the standard deviation is 36.11. Table 6: Mean Perception Scores for the Site Risk Factors (and Standard Deviations) Risk Factor Rank Perceived Risk Level The Huron/Great Miami River 2 4.377 (1.509) Respondent's Home 1 4.698 (1.671) Town Population 5 4.075 (1.627) Chemical Dispersal within Town 4 4.170 (1.477) Tank Car Derailrnent 3 4.264 (1.778) Demographic Profile The demographic profile of the respondents revealed several similarities between the two study areas. The median ages Of the survey respondents is quite similar (45 and 48). While Dexter Village had more females than males respond, Miamisburg had a greater number of males respond. The education and income levels were distributed fairly evenly with the largest discrepancies in the SOME COLLEGE and GRADUATE SCHOOL categories. The education and income levels were comparable to those indicated by all Miamisburg and Dexter Village respondents in the 1990 United States Census. The racial composition of the respondents was 100% Caucasian in Dexter Village and 96% in Miamisburg, which were figures comparable to the 1990 Census profiles. 43 Responses were returned fiom all forty geographic zones (20 in each study area). The questions at the end of the Background section concerned one's involvement in environmental activities, awareness of hazardous material transportation in their community, and the efi‘ectiveness of elected officials. Twenty-two percent of the Dexter Village respondents indicated involvement in an environmental organization, while sixty-six percent of Miarrrisburg respondents stated involvement. In Dexter Village, nineteen percent of the respondents stated that they were aware of Conrail transporting hazardous materials and eleven percent suggested that elected Officials were “actively pursuing ways to mitigate" transportation incidents. Seventy-one percent of the Miamisburg respondents were living in Miamisburg at the time of the derailment, while sixty-six percent were evacuated. Thirty-eight percent of the Miamisburg respondents felt that the elected officials were "actively pursuing ways to rrritigate" transportation incidents. Demographic Issues The issues of gender, education, length of residence, and proximity to the rail were identified by the respondents in section IV of the survey instrument. These data were then incorporated into the analysis (as the independent variables) for testing the hypothesized 44 Table 7: Demographic Profile of Survey Respondents Dexter Village, MI Miamisburg, OH Number of Responses 27 25 Age (Median) 45 48 Sex Female (65%) Female (44%) Male (35%) Male (56%) Education Some High School 4% 8% High School/GED 22% 16% Some College 18% 40% College Completed 26% 28% Technical School 4% 4% Graduate School 26% 4% Length of Residence 6- 10 Years 6—10 Years Race 100% Caucasian 96% Caucasian 4% Native American Income $0- 7,000 0% 4% $7,000-15,000 7% 16% $15,000-22,000 15% 12% 322,000-3 0,000 1 1% 24% $30,000-40,000 15% 16% $40,000-50,000 30% 8% $50,000-60,000 7% 16% $60,000 and greater 15% 4% relationships. The gender, education, and length of residence were indicated on the questionnaire, while the proximity to the rail was tracked through my initial geO-coding process. W2 Before proceeding with t-test and regression analysis to explore the hypothesized relationships presented in the first chapter, I calculated bivariate correlation coemcients between the independent variables (EDUC, PROXIMITY, and LENGTH). TOWN and 45 SEX were not included because they were Dummy variables. The purpose of calculating correlation coefiicients is to identify associations and to assess the potential for multicollinearity among the variables. As Table 8 shows, there is little cause to be concerned about multicollinearity since the highest correlation (between LENGTH and EDUC) is -.221. Table 8 : Bivariate Correlation Coefficients of LENGTH, EDUC, and PROXIMITY LENGTH EDUC PROXIMITY LENGTH 1 .00 EDUC -.221 l .00 PRO)GMITY .051 .124 1.00 Preliminary analysis of my individual ENVIRO, TRANS, and SITE scores produced high correlation values. The correlation values between ENVIRO, TRANS, and SITE suggest a statistically significant relationship exists between the three variables. This multicollinearity indicates that the combination of these variables may be used to test the hypothesis presented in Chapter 1. Due to this high correlation, I proceeded with the TOTAL value in my hypothesis testing. .In addressing the hypothesis that I presented in Chapter I, I used regression and t-test analysis to test the significance of the relationship between the risk perception levels and socioeconomic factors. A t-test is used to compare the two study areas for the hypothesized differences. Chi-Square analysis could also be used in studies of 46 Table 9: Bivariate Correlation Coefiicients of ENVIRO, TRANS, and SITE ENVIRO TRANS SITE ENVIRO 1 .00 TRANS .757 l .00 SITE .623 .823 1.00 comparison between two groups, but it is not the most usefirl test if the sample sizes are small. Regression analysis was used to better understand what, if any, relationships existed between the variables and risk perceptions. The first hypothesis concerns the total risk perception scores of Mianrisburg and Dexter Village, while the last four hypotheses concern the independent variables (SEX, EDUC, PROXIMITY, and LENGTH.) Hypothesis 1: The residents of Miamisburg, Ohio will indicate a higher total degree of risk perception concerning transportable hazardous materials than their Dexter Village, Michigan, counterparts. Due to Miamisburg's past, I surmised that the respondents of Miamisburg would indicate higher levels of risk for all sets of issues. By comparing the two areas, I attempted to difl‘erentiate between the levels of risk perceived. I turned to difference of means t-test analysis for firrther explanation if a difference in total risk levels existed between the two study areas. By placing the TOTAL variable into a t-test, I attempted to distinguish between the two study areas and establish Miamisburg as having a higher level of risk. 47 Table 10: Difference of Means T-Test between Miamisburg and Dexter Village Respondents' TOTAL Scores Mean Standard Deviation Miamisburg 145.727 21.401 Dexter Village 135.706 26.372 Separate Variances DF= 26 Prob“: .302 = 1.054 The difl‘erence-of-means t-test has a calculated t (1.054) less than the critical t at the alpha = .05 level; therefore, I can not accept the hypothesis that the Miamisburg respondents perceive higher risk levels than their Dexter Village counterparts. Prior to applying statistical tests, I suggested that the Miamisburg respondents had a heightened sense of environmental awareness due to the 1986 derailment, but upon completing this t-test I must indicate that this difference is not present. Hypothesis 2: Female respondents will indicate higher levels of perceived risk than the male respondents. In chapter 11, I discussed the link between gender and perceived risk level. I suggested that women would show higher perceived levels of risk due to their greater sense of environmental responsibility. Again, a difference of means t-test was calculated for the combined male and female respondents. This test was used to see if one gender displayed a higher level of perceived risk over the other. 48 Table 11: Difference of Means T-Test between Female and Male Respondents TOTAL Scores Mean Standard Deviation Male 114.00 42.922 Women 139.643 24.629 Separate Variances DF= 49 Prob.= .010 T: 2.674 This t-test has a calculated t value which is larger than the critical t at the alpha = .05 level. Due to the larger calculated t value, I must conclude that female respondents will indicate higher levels of risk than the male respondents. This analysis is consistent with the literature introduced in Chapter II. Hypothesis 3: Education is a significant indicator of perceived risk level, as those with higher levels will perceive greater risk than those with less formal education. As mentioned in Chapter H, past research has questioned whether those with higher levels of education are more environmentally conscious than those less schooled. I used a regression model relating TOTAL to EDUC to see if any relationship exists. By regressing TOTAL and EDUC, I have attempted to model a relationship between an overall risk concern and the respondent's level of education. The regression model explains only 3% of the variance and is not statistically significant at the set alpha= .05 level. 49 Because there are other variables that may afi‘ect this relationship, I used a multiple regression model with TOTAL, EDUC, and TOWN. I then proceeded with a model that included TOTAL, EDUC, TOWN, and ET. The incorporation of the TOWN variable allows for any relationship between the two study areas, education, and risk level to be studied. VVrthin this intermediate model the EDUC variable is significant, but only 5% of the variance is explained and the overall model is not significant. Table 12: Regression Model of TOTAL and EDUC Dependent Variable: TOTAL Multiple R: .226 Multiple R2: .051 Adjusted Multiple R2: .030 Standard Error of Estimate: 35.847 Coefl' B T P Constant 109.336 0.000 7.580 .000 EDUC 5.873 .226 1.571 .123 SS. Df M.S. F P Regression 3169.991 1 3169.991 2.467 .123 Residual 59110.988 46 1285.021 I incorporated the ET variable within this model to ascertain if any discrepancies between education, risk level, and town did exist. This regression explains 13% of the variance within the model, while being significant at the alpha =.05 level. The EDUC and TOWN coefficients are negative slants indicating that the education levels and town 50 Table 13: Regression of TOTAL, EDUC, and TOWN Dependent Variable: TOTAL Multiple R: .304 Multiple R2: .092 Adjusted Multiple R2: .052 Standard Error of Estimate: 35.440 Coefl‘ B Tol. T P Constant 127.425 0.000 6.828 .000 EDUC 7.097 .273 .949 1 .871 .068 TOWN -15.092 -.209 .949 -l .426 .158 SS. Df M.S. F P Regression 5760.566 2 2880.283 2.293 .1 13 Residual 56520.41 45 1256.009 designation are not positively related indicators of the total risk level. Due to the negative direction of the coefiicients, I can not accept the hypothesis even though the model is significant at the alpha = .05 level. Within this model are two equations, one for each town. Both slopes for the towns are negative and Dexter Village's slope is greater than Miamisburg. This negative slope indicates that the influence of education is more pronounced in Dexter Village than in Miamisburg. 51 Table 14: Regression of TOTAL, EDUC, TOWN, and ET Dependent Variable: TOTAL Multiple R: .441 Multiple R2: .194 Adjusted Multiple R2: .139 Standard Error of Estimate: 33.774 Coefl‘ B Tol. T P Constant 223.417 0.000 7.580 .000 EDUC -22.024 -.846 .075 -1.710 .094 TOWN -75.151 -1.042 .127 -2.743 .009 ET 17.749 .570 .041 2.356 .023 SS. Df M.S. F P Regression 12090.133 3 4030.044 3.533 .022 Residual 50190.845 44 1140.701 Hypothesis 4: Proximity to the rail is significantly related to perceived risk, and those living closer to the rail will perceive higher levels of risk than those at a further distance fiom the railway. After establishing the risk exposure area of two miles for both study areas, I can incorporate the relationship between distance and risk levels. The proximity of the respondents to the railway is expected to increase their perceived risk levels due to the heightened concern for a deraihnent or dispersal. The regression model of TOTAL and PROXIMITY explains less than one percent of the variance and at the alpha=.05 level the overall model is not significant. From this point, I will integrate the TOWN variable into the TOTAL and PROXIMITY regression 52 Table 15: Regression Model of TOTAL and PROXIMITY Dependent Variable: TOTAL Multiple R: .078 Multiple R2: .006 Adjusted Multiple R2: .000 Standard Error of Estimate: 36.370 Coeff B T P Constant 122.813 0.00 1 1.293 .000 PROXIMITY 8.199 0.00 .548 .586 SS. Df M.S. F P Regression 397.097 1 .097 .300 .586 Residual 64814.589 49 1322.747 model allowing for any distinctions between study area. The regression model of TOTAL, PROXIMITY, and TOWN explained only 5% of the variance, low tolerance levels signaling some multicollinearity, and is not significant overall. Both of the independent variables are statistically significant. The opposite Of the hypothesis is revealed in this model, as the TOTAL risk is increasing with the distance from the railroad within the range Of study in both towns. The starting risk level is less in TOWN=1, but the risk increases in each as the distance increases (See Figure 4) . The idea that proximity is related to risk levels and breeds contentment is supported with these results. I will next incorporate the PT variable to see if a relationship exists when these variables are controlled. 53 Table 16: Regression Model of TOTAL, PROXIMITY, and TOWN Dependent Variable: TOTAL Multiple R: .302 Multiple R2: .091 Adjusted Multiple R2: .053 Standard Error of Estimate: 35.137 Coefl‘ B Tol. T P Constant 147.425 0.00 9.417 .000 PROXIMITY 31.880 .303 .626 1.746 .087 TOWN -26.375 -.369 .626 -2. 121 .039 8.8. Df M.S. F P Regression 5949.393 2 2974.697 2.409 . 101 Residual 59262.293 48 1234.631 This regression explains less than one percent of the variance within the model and has only one variable within the model (TOWN). The tolerance levels are again low indicating that multicollinearity exists within the model. The dummy variable (PT) allows for a regression plane for each town, yet it is not statistically significant within this model. The overall model is insignificant (p=.124.) The TOWN variable is significant yet the slant of the slope is negative. The results presented in the previous model concerning the increase in risk levels with distance is negated when incorporating the TOWN dummy (PT). The increase in risk is at roughly similar rates in the two towns, but the starting position of Town=1 is lower (see Figure 4). Although the significant relationship is not related to the hypothesis it is worthy of note. Due to the irrsignificance of both models, I can not accept the hypothesis that those living closer to the rail will perceive greater levels 54 Table 17: Regression Model Of TOTAL, PROXIMITY, TOWN, and PT Dependent Variable: TOTAL Multiple R: .078 Multiple R2: .006 Adjusted Multiple R2: .000 Standard Error of Estimate: 36.370 Coeff B Tol. T P Constant 186.987 0.00 4.781 .000 PROXIMITY -48.766 -.464 .037 -.647 .521 TOWN -51.120 -.715 .147 -l.994 .052 PT 46.191 1.028 .022 1.103 .276 SS. Df M.S. F P Regression 7445 .416 3 2481.805 2.019 . 124 Residual 57766.27] 47 1229.070 of risk than those further away. The proximity of the rail lines to the residents in both communities is very close and there may have been too little variation to detect any relationships. Figure 4: Comparison of TOTAL and PT Slope/Intercept Values TOTAL TOTAL = 147.425+3l.88*PROXIMITY-26.375*TOWN Intercept =147.425 TOWN = l / TOWN = 0 Intercept = 121.05 PROXIMITY 55 Hypothesis 5: The length of residence is a significant indicator of risk levels as respondents with greater lengths of residence will indicate higher levels of risk than those with shorter length of residence. The length of residence can be influential on the perceived level Of risk in that the greater the length of residence, the greater the level of risk that will be perceived. This should be evident in Miamisburg, as those who lived there ten years age may indicate higher levels of risk due to their recollections and expoSure to the derailment. Table 18: Regression of TOTAL and LENGTH Dependent Variable: TOTAL Multiple R: .320 Multiple R2: .102 Adjusted Multiple R2: .083 Standard Error Of Estimate: 34.340 Coefi B T P Constant 150.311 0.00 14.550 .000 LENGTH -6.964 -0.320 -2.337 .024 SS. Df M.S. F P Regression 6441.989 1 6441.989 5.463 .024 Residual 56604.011 48 1179.250 A regression model using TOTAL and LENGTH was executed to assess whether the length of one's residence was influential in the level of risk perceived by the respondent. The overall model was significant, yet the coefficient for the LENGTH variable is a negative slant. This negative slant may suggest that the greater one's length 56 of residence, the lower one's perceived risk levels due to the continued transportation of such materials without any type of mishap. This may be descriptive of Dexter Village's attitudes as the residents did not harbor any personal experiences with hazardous materials. Again, this does not support the hypothesis. At this point, I can not accept the hypothesis that the greater the length of time a respondent resides in the area, the higher the perceived level of risk. I have continued to model a regression to see if any differences exist between the two study areas. For the next model, I incorporated the TOTAL, LENGTH, and TOWN variables into a regression. By adding the dummy variable (TOWN) into the model, I can assess whether any differences exist between the Dexter Village and Mianrisburg respondents regarding risk level and length of residence. Table 19: Regression of TOTAL, LENGTH, and TOWN Dependent Variable: TOTAL Multiple R: .410 Multiple R2: .168 Adjusted Multiple R2: .114 Standard Error of Estimate: 33.760 Coefi‘ B Tol. T P Constant 170.500 0.00 9.373 0.00 LENGTH -7 .208 -.331 .996 -2.435 .119 TOWN -12.962 -. 183 .996 -1.343 .186 88. Df M.S. F P Regression 8534.140 2 4267.070 3 .679 .033 Residual 54511.860 47 1 159.827 57 Figure 5: Comparison of TOTAL and LENGTH Slope/Intercept Values TOTAL = 170.500 - 7.208 * LENGTH - 12.962 * TOWN TOTAL Intercept = 170.500 VN=0 Intercept = 157.538 TOWN = 1 LENGTH The tolerance levels within this model are very high and the overall model is statistically significant; yet, the independent variables Of LENGTH and TOWN were not significant. Less than 12% ofthe variance was explained in this model. Again, I will incorporate another dummy variable into the model (LT) which describes the length of residence multiplied by the town dummy variable. If the LT were to be significant, the variable would describe the two study areas on different regression planes (See Figure 5). This regression explains less than 12% Of the variance within the model but is statistically significant at the alpha = .05 level. None of the independent variables within the model are significant. The two towns have the same slope yet TOWN=O has a higher intercept suggesting that as length increases the TOTAL risk level decreases. Although this finding is not in the direction of the hypothesis, it is worthy of note. 58 Table 20: Regression of TOTAL, LENGTH, TOWN, and LT Dependent Variable: TOTAL Multiple R: .410 Multiple R2: .168 Adjusted Multiple R2: .114 Standard Error of Estimate: 33.760 Coefl‘ B Tol. T Constant 126.367 0.00 3.389 LENGTH 6.933 .318 .073 .638 TOWN 13.206 .186 .196 .612 LT -8.456 -.749 .059 -l.352 S.S. Df M.S. F Regression 10618.701 3 3539.567 3.106 Residual 52427 .299 46 1 139.724 Table 21: Review of Hypothesis Outcomes 1. Miamisburg Risk Levels > Dexter Village Risk Level Reject 2. Females will indicate higher levels of risk than Males Accept 3. Higher Education Levels = Higher Risk Levels Reject (the opposite prevailed) 4. Closer Proximity to the rail = Higher Risk Levels Reject 5. Greater Length of Residence = Higher Risk Levels Reject .000 .526 .544 .183 .036 Chapter V: Discussion The Objectives Of this study were to measure the perceived risks in two communities, assess any relationships, and to discuss if demographic factors were influential in the perception process. The study's hypotheses were used to propose that socioeconomic factors were influential in how risk is perceived and that one of the communities would indicate higher levels of risk due to their past experience with a train derailment. Past risk perception research has focused on the controversies surrounding facility siting and disposal. Awareness of industry's past practices has created increased regulatory oversight and community concern over potential environmental impacts. Anxiety over disposal practices has focused on what long term threats are posed by hazardous materials not only to the natural environment but to human health. The purpose of this study was to analyze and assess risks perceived by two Midwestern towns in association with the transportation of hazardous materials and to determine if these groups differed in their perceptions. Dexter Village, Michigan, and Miamisburg, Ohio, were chosen for a comparative assessment of two areas with similar socioeconomic compositions and are currently encountering hazardous materials traveling through their limits. 59 60 The study results established a ranking of risk levels which were used to test the hypothesis presented in the first chapter. The results did not concur with all of the stated hypotheses. The perceived levels of risk were much more similar than dissimilar between the two study areas. However, the results did indicate a consensus among respondents that hazardous materials transportation mishaps are something that those in planning positions should work to mitigate. Literature cited in this study established the concern for hazardous material processes including treatment, storage, and disposal. Whereas Slovic has looked at both siting issues in the Yucca Mountains and highway transportation in Oregon, this study identified the risks perceived with respect to transporting hazardous materials by rail. The use of risk factors either jointly or individually, such as the highest risk to community water supplies (Great Miami and Huron Rivers) and rail conditions, can be targeted by planners as those areas in which to better inform the public. Risk perception as an additional facet in the planning process is vital for the continuation of effective policy. The necessity for local communities to capably handle the issues facing their residents and to have intact contingency plans in the event of a mishap is becoming more evident. Miamisburg experienced this type of incident first hand and witnessed the ramifications of the lack of communication between Local, State, and Federal officials. 61 Qpnplpsions and Recommengtipps The commencement of this project brought several ideas and generalizations about how the respondents of these two communities may react and perceive risks associated with the transporting of hazardous materials. I knew the history of the Miamisburg area, of the ramifications of the derailment, and the attitudes toward the DOE Mound facility. I knew very little about Dexter Village beyond the fact that it was a quaint town tucked outside of Ann Arbor. Both towns had one commonality though, that rail trafic is going through their communities carrying hazardous materials. The conclusions drawn fiom this study were surprising. The degree to which the two communities agreed on the highest and lowest risks to their communities was hypothesized to be significantly different. I expected to see more dissension than agreement. I suspected that Miamisburg respondents would indicate extremely high levels of risk throughout the survey while Dexter Village might have more variations. Instead, the study presented a high degree of agreement between Dexter Village and Miamisburg. Following is a more detailed discussion of the study’s conclusions. It must be acknowledged, however, that there may have been serious problems with the clarity of the questionnaire that limits these conclusions. Caperal Environmental Isspes The overall ranking of the general environmental issues by both towns were shown to be relatively similar. The ranking of the highest risk to human health "Toxic Air Pollutants" and "Industrial Discharges" as the highest risk to the environment were 62 established concerns demonstrated by the respondents. I suggest that the air pollutants and discharges were perceived as the highest levels in Miamisburg due to the phosphorous cloud from the derailment and the current remediation ongoing at the DOE Mound facility. Dexter Village respondents may have indicated air pollutants and discharges due to the unknown nature of the materials. Air pollution is an unknown risk to the average human due to the relative invisibility Of the materials. Industrial discharges conjure up images of Love Canal and the threat of toxins polluting our water supplies and homes. This study analyzed two Midwestern towns on the basis of their perception of risk in transporting hazardous materials. The similarities that exist could be fostered by the media and the increasing attention given toward environmental hazards. The highest risk levels identified by the respondents could be addressed by community planners and implemented into community educational planning. Transportation Risk Factors Shierbl identified transportation risk factors in his study of inter-agency perception regarding the transportation of hazardous materials (1994). By using the transportation risk factors in this study, I could assess the perception of risks associated with the transportation process. The ranking of risk factors was more diverse in this section. The respondents identified "Condition of the Rail and Tank Cars" as the highest risk factor, while "The Time of Day when Rail Shipments are Transported" was chosen the least risky. The recognition Of rail conditions as the most risky transportation factor is understandable in Miamisburg, as the deraihnent was caused by a kink in the tracks. The 63 Dexter Village respondents concern for the rail condition is natural. Ultimately that is the most important factor in the transporting of any material over any distance. The need for safe, reliable rail routes is a must for the continued transportation of hazardous materials. The "Time of Day" as being the least risk is not surprising, as very few people ever interact with rail transportation. Most rail routes have very few public crossing points; therefore, the old cliché' "Out of Sight, Out of Mind" is the sentiment of most respondents. An interesting aside though is that a few Dexter respondents expressed concern over the trains going through the Village at night. For highway transport, many communities have time periods in which materials may be transported (e. g. not during school hours), but I do not know of any current or firture plans for time designations for transporting by rail in either Miamisburg or Dexter Village. Site Risk Factors By choosing two study areas with comparable topography, I could assess perceptions dealing with site specific factors which were in both towns. The respondents of both Dexter Village and Mianrisburg indicated that their own "homes" were at the greatest risk. This ranking is due to the proximity of the towns' population densities to the rail (side by side.) I suggest that this concern was also conjured on the fear of the unknown in Dexter Village, as the threat of something infiltrating our private homes is relatively scary. The high number of Miamisburg respondents who indicated that they were in Miamisburg during the derailment had experience with this type of risk and therefore indicated high risk levels. 64 The respondents' indicated that the "town population" was at the lowest risk from any type of incident. I suggest that this is due to the evacuation of the entire town in Miamisburg and the surrounding communities after the derailment. The thinking behind this lower level risk may be that the "town population" is all encompassing and most spills or accidents would likely be smaller scale. The relevance of the site risk factors can be attributed to the awareness of the vulnerability of the communities. Miamisburg is consistently given as an example, but the 1986 derailment demonstrated how a mishap can disrupt everyday life and possibly cause long term effects. This type of analysis and recognition of local issues is instrumental in developing contingency plans within the community. Hypotheses The hypotheses presented in Chapter I were statistically tested in Chapter IV. Of the five hypotheses presented, I could only accept one. My hypotheses were based on the socioeconomic/perception findings of past literature. The literature basis of environmental risk perception is still in the infant stages, but I submit this study's results as a basis for further research. Prior to executing this research, I had definite ideas about the risk awareness in each of the study areas. I anticipated Miamisburg having a heightened level and Dexter Village exhibiting some awareness. The testing of my hypotheses dispelled this preliminary thinking. The education and length of residence relationships that I hypothesized were actually the opposite of the analyzed results. The length variable indicated a negative slope within the regression model suggesting that one may negate any 65 types of risk due to the continued transportation of materials through the communities with little or no problem. The proximity variable within my regression models suggested that the risk levels began to increase as the distance began to increase indicating that those further from the transportation corridors may view transportation as more risky. The broad range of environmental activities in communities today, such as recycling, has created a greater awareness than may have been anticipated. The demographic relationships that I suggested were also not statistically significant. Willem Upon completion of this study, I began to reflect on the overall structure of the study and address issues that could be clarified for any firture attempts at perception studies. The need for perception studies remains vital for the formulation of public policy; therefore, the methods that reveal public attitudes clearly and accurately are crucial. The urve The success of a perception study is dependent on the responses received. If I were to attempt another study analogous to this one, I would implement a survey distribution that would involve more person-to-person interaction. The theory is that the more personal interaction, the greater the number of completed responses (Dillman 1978). This interaction can be achieved by telephone interviews or focus groups. With the advent of telemarketing, the telephone is viewed as one route to survey a representative sample about the issue of choice. The advantages of this type of survey 66 include immediate responses, a clarification of any questions, and the ability to better inform the respondent of any questions or concerns regarding the survey. The disadvantages of a phone survey are the expense (long-distance), time intensive nature, and the potential need for additional staff, not to mention the disruption of activity for the respondents. The telephone method would appear to be quite harmless, but I suggest that the intrusive manner of phone surveying may not result in a greater number of completed responses. This method is usefiil but may not be the best approach for perception studies. The Focus Group method is one technique that is commonly used by governmental entities for environmental policy creation. The groupa generally include a mixture of citizen action groups, municipalities, technical people, industry, university Officials, and state agencies. One example of the use of such group in an environmental study is the effort by the Ohio Low-Level Radioactive Waste Facility Development Authority. The Authority currently uses a Focus Group in their attempt to successfully site a Low-Level Radioactive Waste Facility within Ohio. Within a perception studies context, a focus group could utilize a representative sample of both study areas and survey their perceptions of risks from transportable hazardous waste. The advantages of the Focus Group include clarification of questions, immediate responses, and a better understanding of the project and what is to be gained by such an undertaking. The disadvantages are the difficulty in assembling a representative sample, scheduling conflicts, and potential lack of interest. For any firture perception analysis, I suggest that the focus group method may yield the most results. 67 One additional survey method that could be used for perception studies is that of the web page. The dramatic increase in Internet usage has established another route by which a survey can be distributed. By creating a web site, virtually anyone with a computer and internet service can log on and complete a survey. This method is very useful when demographics are not a component of the study and it is also inexpensive. Qupsp'pnnaire Struflre The creation of any future perception studies would result in various changes in my survey instrument. The questionnaire that I distributed in Dexter Village and Miamisburg included Likert style scaling with closed-ended questions. This type of format is useful in gauging perceptions, but one person may indicate a "High" response as a "4" while another respondent may consider a "High" perception level a "6. " The respondents would then perceive the risk at the same level but indicate two difi'erent numbers of risk. The more appropriate scale may be: NO Risk, Low Risk, Medium Risk, High Risk, Extreme Risk, perhaps comparing to standards (being struck by lightening, being in an auto accident, etc.) This scale would allow the respondent to identify the wording with their exact perception level. At the very least, some questions should be included that allow comparison of levels. For example, there is no reason to expect a difi‘erence between Miamisburg and Dexter Village on perception of risk of the United States facing some particular environmental problems. Other values could then be scaled relative to the individual's average values on those items. 68 I suggest, too, that various wording changes be made before re-administering this type of perception question. I submit that I may have created directions and environmental risk factors that may have been difficult to understand for the average lay person. The directions could be stated, "Given your understanding of how potentially hazardous materials are currently handled and regulated, what level of risk do you believe to be associated with: Air pollutants from industry, Industrial Discharges to Surface Water, etc? “With this approach, I may have had more residents respond to the questionnaire rather than discarding them. A questionnaire that is clearer and involves more risk factors within the study area will probably yield higher response rates and an overall interest within the study areas. Table 22 is a shortened, revised sample. The "NO Risk to Extreme Risk" scale can be converted to a numerical scale (1 through 5) for input into statistical analysis. The same conversion can take place for the percentage likelihood (between .00 and 1.00). The demographic section of the questionnaire is included to ascertain what socioeconomic variables may correlate to certain perceived risk levels. I included in this survey's questionnaire common questions concerning Racial Identification, Household Composition, and Yearly Household Income. The inclusion of such categories may have been a deterrent for some potential respondents because this type Of information is very personal. I did not need to include the Racial Identification since I knew that both study areas are almost completely homogeneous. I did include the Income and Household composition categories though to assess what relationships may exist between risk levels 69 and these variable. Due to the lack of responses containing this information, I decided to withdraw this type of analysis fi'om the research project. An additional facet that I would address for firture perception studies is the distance from which to survey an exposure area. The wide-spread use of the information super highway and twenty-four hour news networks creates a broader exposure for the dissemination of news. For example, the 1986 Miamisburg derailment was broadcast in Southern Ohio and probably few other places. The 1996 Wisconsin derailment was broadcast on the National networks. Due to this increased exposure, I would try to assess perceptions at greater distances from the study areas. For the Dexter Village scenario, I would distribute surveys in Ann Arbor, Brighton, Metropolitan Detroit, and Jackson. The Miamisburg exposure area would include Dayton, Springfield, Columbus, and Cincinnati. I would also attempt some random surveys throughout the United States. 70 Table 22: Sample of Reconstructed Survey Part 1: Environmental Issues Concerning Hazardous Waste Transportation Given your understanding of how potentially hazardous materials are currently handled and regulated, what level of risk do you believe to be associated with: Risk flsk to Human Health 1. Air Pollutants from Industry NO Risk Low Medium High Extreme 2. Industrial Discharges to Surface Water No Risk Low Medium High Extreme 3. Contaminants from Hazardous Waste NO Risk Low Medium High Extreme Storage Sites 4 Drinking Water fi'om Tap NO Risk Low Medium High Extreme M Risk to the Envirpnment 5. Air Pollutants from Industry NO Risk Low Medium High Extreme 6. Industrial Discharges to Surface Water NO Risk Low Medium High Extreme 7. Contaminants from Hazardous Waste Storage Sites Drinking Water from Tap No Risk Low Medium High Extreme .°° No Risk Low Medium High Extreme Part II: Hazardous Materials Transportation The following is a list Of risk factors attributable to hazardous material transportation. What level of risk do you believe to be associated with each factor: M Risk Level 1. Volume of Materials Transported No Risk Low Medium High Extreme 2. Distance Materials Are Transported No Risk Low Medium High Extreme 3. Condition of the Rail and Tank Cars NO Risk Low Medium High Extreme 4. Weather Conditions During Transport No Risk Low Medium High Extreme 5. Population Density Along Railroad No Risk Low Medium High Extreme 6. Natural Resources Along Railroad NO Risk Low Medium High Extreme 7. Emergency Response Along Railroad NO Risk Low Medium High Extreme 8. Type of Chemicals Transported No Risk Low Medium High Extreme 9. Time of Day During Transport No Risk Low Medium High Extreme 10. Chemical Storage During Transport No Risk Low Medium High Extreme 71 Part III: Hazardous Materials Within Dexter Village/Miamisburg Given your understanding of the amount, frequency, and type of hazardous materials moving through your community by rail, what do you think is the likelihood of an accident harmfirl to each of the following in the next ten years. Give any value fiom 0% to 100%. R_is__k Likelihood 1. The Huron/Great Miami River 2. Your Home 3. The Dexter Village/Miamisburg Population What do you think is the likelihood of the following: 4. A Chemical Dispersal In Dexter Village/Miamisburg 5. Evacuation fi'om your Home Part IV: Background Information Age Sex: M F Length of Residence: Years Educational Level Attained: Months Less Than High School High School or Equivalent Some College College Completed Technical School Graduate School Are you or any member of your household currently active in any environmental organizations? Y N (Dexter Village only) Are you aware of Conrail transporting hazardous materials through Dexter Village? Y N (Miamisburg only) Did you live in Miamisburg during the 1986 derailment? Y N Were you Evacuated? Y N Do you feel the elected and regulatory Officials are actively pursuing ways to mitigate any type of transportation incident which might involve hazardous materials? Y N 72 My reflection of this study has suggested some areas I would change if this study were repeated. I suggest that perception studies, such as this, are vital to the ever evolving creation Of environmental public policy. As the volume of perception study continues to increase, more literature will prevail suggesting new socioeconomic variables that may influence perceptions. I submit this study to the growing volume of literature and look forward to the attention that hazardous materials transportation risks will continue to receive. APPENDIX A 73 MlCHlCAN STATE U N I V E R S | T Y Dear Respondent: The Department of Geography at Michigan State University is conducting an exploratory study of risks associated with the transportation of hazardous wastes on Ohio and Michigan railroads. This questionnaire is being administered to a cross section Of Dexter Village, Michigan, and Miarrrisburg, Ohio, inhabitants in order to learn about and suggest improvements in the way hazardous waste transportation programs and policies are implemented in both states. It will take approximately 15 nrinutes to complete this questionnaire. A report describing the study's results and potential implications for public policy will be generated from the Obtained information and available for anyone to review. Responses to the questionnaire will be analyzed on a group basis only and will remain totally confidential. Please return the completed questionnaire in the envelope included in this package by Friday, September 1, 1995. Thank you for your time and assistance in this study. Your participation is essential in obtaining useful results. Sincerely, WW5 Beth A Myers Graduate Student W494i @3032}? Dr. Bruce Pigozzi Department of Geography Project Advisor APPENDIX B 74 MICHIGAN STATE UNIVERSITY September 9, 1995 Dear Respondent: One month ago a questionnaire asking for your views on the transportation of hazardous materials by rail through Dexter Village/Miamisburg was distributed to you. If you have completed the questionnaire already, please accept our sincere thanks. If not, could you please return it today? The questionnaire was sent to only a small representative sample; therefore, it is important that your views are included in this study if we are to represent the people Of Dexter Village’s/ Miamisburg's views accurately. . If you did not receive a questionnaire or have mislaid it, I have enclosed another copy and stamped envelope within this packet. Sincerely, WWVé’ Beth A. Myers Graduate Student Dr. Bruce Pigozzi Department of Geography Project Advisor APPENDIX C 75 Part 1. Environmental Issues Concerning Hazardous Waste Transportation A. Below are issues pertinent to the environmental concern and the subsequent transportation of hazardous materials. Please circle the number that best describes you personal understanding of each issue and if it is deemed a risk either to the environment or to human health. The response scale ranges from O= NO Risk to 6: High Risk. Risk to Risk to Risk Factor Human Health the Environnrent None--Medium--High None--Medium--High 1.Airpollutantsincluding 0123456 0123456 sulfur dioxide, total suspended particulates, carbon monoxide, nitrogen, oxides, ozones, and lead. 2. Toxic air pollutants including dioxin 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 O 1 2 3 4 5 6 3. Industrial dischargestosurface water 0 l 2 3 4 5 6 O l 2 3 4 5 6 4. Contaminantsrcleasedfrom 0 l 2 3 4 5 6 O l 2 3 4 5 6 hazardous waste sites that are actively being managed 5. Accidental releases of chemicals 0 l 2 3 4 5 6 0 l 2 3 4 5 6 6. Radiationfromsourcesotherthan O l 2 3 4 5 6 0 l 2 3 4 5 6 indoor radon 7. Drinking water fi'om tap O 1 2 3 4 5 6 O 1 2 3 4 5 6 8. Workerexposuretochemicals 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 0 l 2 3 4 5 6 76 B. Part H. Hazardous Materials Transportation The following is a list of risk factors attributable to hazardous material transportation. Please circle the number that best describes your personal feeling of the overall "risk" represented by each issue. The response scale ranges from 0=No Risk to 6=High Risk. ie. The following question should be read as, "The type of materials being transported represent which level of risk?" Risk NO Risk High Risk 1. Thetypeofmaterialsbeingtransported . 0 l 2 3 4 5 6 2. Volumeofmaterialsbeingtransported 0 l 2 3 4 5 6 3. Emergency response capability along transport route. 0 l 2 3 4 5 6 4. Distancethehazardous materialsarebeingtransported O 1 2 3 4 5 6 5. Population density along the railroad. O l 2 3 4 5 6 6. Natural resource density andtypealong the railroad. 0 l 2 3 4 5 6 7. Conditionoftherailandtankcars. O l 2 3 4 5 6 8. The time of day when rail shipments are transported. 0 l 2 3 4 5 6 9. Weather conditions during transport. 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 10.Typeofstoragewithintankcarduringtransport. 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 III. Hazardous Material Transportation within Dexter Village/Miamisburg The following is a list of risk factors attributable to hazardous material transportation by railroad within Dexter Village/Miamisburg. Please circle the number that best describes your personal feeling of the overall "risk" represented by each issue. The response scale ranges fiom 0=No Risk to 6=High Risk. Risk No Risk High Risk 1. TheHuron/GreatMiamiRiver 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 2. The respondent's home 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 3. The Dexter Village/Miamisburg Population 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 4. Chemical dispersal in Dexter Village/Miamisburg O l 2 3 4 5 6 5. Tank car derailment or accident 0 1 2 3 4 S 6 77 Part IV. Background Age Children Sex: M F Length Of Residence: Years Educational Level Attained: Months Some High School High School or Equivalent Some College College Completed Technical School Graduate School Racial Identification: Yearly Household Income Afiican-American $0-7,000 Asian $7, 000-1 5,000 Caucasian $15,000-22,000 Latino $22,000-30,000 Native American $30,000-40,000 “0,000-50,000 $50,000-60,000 $60,000 and greater Are you or any member of your household currently active in any environmental organizations? Y N Are you aware Of Conrail transporting hazardous materials through Dexter Village? Y N ‘ Did you live in Miamisburg during the 1986 derailment? Y N Were you evacuated? Y N Do you feel the elected and regulatory Officials are actively pursuing ways to mitigate any type of transportation incident which might involve hazardous materials? Y N APPENDIX D 78 m r n in ' ' r hi I was working in Miamisburg on July 8, 1986 (train derailment) and had to leave. My family has had lung problems and bad health ever since the derailment, and I believe if officials would have told the public more about the problems with the type of chemicals, but instead all they told us was it was not a health problem. I am in the area that was asked to leave at the time of the derailment, but far enough away to be affected with the smoke drifting my way. Precautionary measures were only temporary after the 1986 derailment, train speed is back up and cherrrical cars are not dispersed throughout the total length of the train. The DOE has NOT been open and honest with Miamisburg residents about CHEMICAL and RADIATION RELEASES from the "former" nuclear weapons research facility. I feel that too much hazardous waste is being transported secretly and it all boils down to money. Companies will ship their waste long distances to avoid the high cost of disposal within their locality. I am more concerned about Mound and what it has done to the water, etc. Also, space crafts going in space are surely not good on ozone either. My belief is that as long as these companies are making money, they don‘t care about the environment. It is really too bad because if they don't start to care about the Earth it may never heal. Why did you ask these questions? ‘ Comments frpm resppndgnts in DQflgt‘ Village, Michigan I was not aware that hazardous materials are being transported so close to my house. I don't like it. It all feels like high risk to me. It's scary. I am concerned that they are producing hazardous waste to begin with- much less moving them around the country. Thanks for the opportunity to fill this out. Until this survey, 1 had not been aware of anything to do with this subject. Most of the freight trains come through at night, as far as I can determine. Those 1 do see during the daylight hours don't seem to have warning signs on the cars. 79 I will not vote for anything that will raise my taxes - most of you- waste our money with high salaries and big committees to do nothing. Local government has taken no interest in the Conrail issue, as the train runs thru Chelsea, Dexter, Ann Arbor, and Ypsilanti. However, the local fire departments have a haz mat team, they also work with Wayne Co. Great Lakes region is especially vulnerable and requires strict protection. Small towns cannot support the infrastructure necessary to deal with a major event. LIST OF REFERENCES LIST OF REFERENCES Arbuthnot, Jack. 1977. " The roles of Attitudinal and Personality Variables in the Prediction of Environmental Behavior and Knowledge". Environment and Behavior 9, 2: 217-231. Arcury, TA. and Scollay, SJ. and TR Johnson. 1987. "Sex Differences in Environmental Concern and Knowledge: The Case of Acid Rain". Sex Roles 16: 463-472. Backstorrrr, Charles H. and Gerald Hursch. 1963. Survgy Rgssargph. Chicago, Illinois: Northwestern University Press. Borden, Richard J., and Janice L. Francis. 1978. "Who Cares about Ecology: Personality and Sex Difi‘erences in Environmental Concern". J oumgl of Personality 46, 1:3 5-39 Borden, Richard J. and Andrew P. Schettino. 1979. "Deterrrrinants of Environmentally Responsible Behavior". Journgl Of Environmental Education 10, 4:35-39. Borsky, RN. 1961. "Community Relations to Air Force Noise-Data on Community Studies and their Interpretation." Dayton, Ohio: Wright-Patterson Air Force Base. Bruvold, William H. 1973. "Belief and Behavior as Determinants of Environmental Attitudes" Environment and Behavior 5, 2: 202-18. Bunting, Trudi E. and Leonard Guelke. 1979. "Behavioral and Perception Geography: A Critical Appraisal." Annals of the American Association of Geographers 69:448-462. Burton, Ian and Robert W. Kates. 1964. "The Perception of Natural Hazards in Resource Management." Natural Resources Joum_a_l. Albuquerque, New Mexico: University of New Mexico School of Law. Buttel, Frederick H. and Donald E. Johnson. 1977. "Dimensions of Environmental Concern: Factor Structures, Correlates, and Implications for Research". Journal of Environmentafl Education 9, 2:49-64. Calef, Wesley. 1950. "The Winter of 1948-1949 in the Great Plains". Annals of the Associapion of Ameripan Geographers. 40: 267. 80 81 Clapharn, W.B., Jr. 1990. "Some Approaches to Assessing the Environmental Risk in Siting Hazardous Waste Facilities". The Environmental Professional 12, 1:32-39. Clarke, Lee. 1989. "Acceptable Risk? Making Decisions in a Toxic Environment". Berkeley: University of California Press. Commoner, Barry. 1988. "Acceptable Risks: Who Decides?" Hagper's Magazine. May 1988. Constantini, Edmond and Kenneth Hanf. 1972. "Environmental Concern and Lake Tahoe". Environment and Behavior June. Craik, Kenneth H. and Ervin H. Zube. 1976. Perceivingéinvironmeml Ouafly. New York: Plenum Press. Crowe, MI. 1968. "Towards a Defirritional Model of Public Perceptions of Air Pollution." Journal of Air Pollution Control Association. 18: 154-157. Cunningham, William P. 1994. Unoorstanding Our Environment. Dubuque, Iowa: Wm. C. Brown Publishing. Dear, Michael. (1992). "Understanding and Overcoming the NIMBY Syndrome." Journal of the Americap Planning Association. 58: 288-292. Dillman, Don A 1978. Mail and Telephone Surveys, The Total Design Method. New York: John Wiley and Sons. Douglas, Mary and Aaron Wildavsky. 1982. Risk and Culture. Berkeley: University of California Press. Dunlap, Riley E. and Kent D. Van Liere. 1978. The "New Environmental Paradigm." Iournal of Environmentol Education 9, 4:10-19. Flora. (1956)1_1gilstorms of the United Stotos_. Goodey, Brian. 1974. Imogos of Plgoo; Essfls on Environmontal Peroeption, Communication and Education. Birmingham, England: The Centre for Urban and Regional Studies. Gregory, Robin and Robert Mendelsohn. 1993. "Perceived Risk, Dread, and Benefits". Risk Analysis 13, 3: 12-19. 82 Kunreuther, Howard and Ruth Patrick. 1991. "Managing the Risks of Hazardous Waste". Environment 13, 4: 21-26. Lowenthal, David. 1967. "Environmental Perception and Behavior" Department Of Geography research paper 109. Chicago, Illinois: University of Chicago. Marsh, Catherine. 1982. The Sowey Method: The Contribution of Surveys to Sociologioaj Explanation. Boston: George Allen and Unwin. McNeil, Sue and Sci-Chang Oh. 1991. "A Note on the Influence Of Rail Defects on the Risk Associated with Shipping Hazardous Materials by Rail". Risk Analysis 11, 2: 14-20. Morgenstem, Richard and Stuart Sessions. 1988. "Weighing Environmental Risks: EPA's Unfinished Business". Environment 30, 6: 14-39. National Transportation Safety Board. 1981. Status Of Department Of Transportation's Hazardous Materials Regolatog Program. Department of Transportation. Noe, Francis P. and Rob Snow. "The New Environmental Paradigm and Further Scale Analysis". Journfl of Environmental Education 26: 20-26. Roberts, L. 1990. "Counting on Science at EPA". Science 2492616. Saccomanno, FF. and 1H. Shortreed. 1993. "Hazmat Transport Risks: Societal and Individual Perspectives". loom Of Transpogtaiion Engineering 119, 2: 177-188. Schahn, Joachim and Erwin Holzer. 1990. " Studies of Individual Environmental Concern". Environment and Behgvior 22, 6: 767-786. Sheskin, Ira M. 1985. " Survey of Research for Geographers". Washington, DC: American Association of Geographers. Slovic, Paul. 1986. "Informing and Educating the Public about Risk". Risk Analysis 6, 4. Slovic, Paul. 1987. "Perception of Risk". Science 236: 280-85. Slovic, Paul, Mark Layman, Nancy Kraus, James Flynn, James Chalmers, and Gail Gessell. 1991. "Perceived Risk, Stigma, and Potential Economic Impacts Of a High Level Nuclear Waste Repository in Nevada". Risk Analysis 11, 4: 683-696. 83 United States Bureau of the Census. 1990. 1990 Michigan State Census. United States Bureau of the Census. 1990. 1990 Ohio State Census. Van Liere, Kent D. and Riley E. Dunlap. 1981. "Environmental Concern". Environment and Behavior 13, 6: 651-675. Wall, G. 1972. "Public Response to Air Pollution in Sheffield". International Geographical Union, Commission on Man and Environment. Calgary Symposium. July 23-31. White, G. 1964. Papers on Flood Problems. Chicago, Illinois: University of Chicago Department of Geography. “v Om "IIIIIIIIIIII