‘ a. - “7C -' - ~' ' h‘ ‘13?“‘4'i * 9'3": Ls}: ! r mam 44: 9" .-. :21 '. “it ‘41 “if: "0 t-~‘#“"' AZ"; V 3‘ I, H ‘ .... ”2;. ‘ . , ' |.ri.." .1" I WWIIWW!!!WIN!!!MINIMUMMINIMUM 01602 4089 LIBRARY Michigan State University PLACE IN RETURN BOX to remove this checkout from your record. TO AVOID FINES return on or before date due. DATE DUE MTE DUE DATE DUE 1M www.mu .-.-I A, 2.235.813 A LIBRARY- ‘ 'Michigan State ; avUniversity . ' ' This is to certify that the I ’ ‘ _ thesis entitled 3 THE STRUCTURE; RELIABILITY AND VALIDITY OF THE AFFECTIVE SENSITIVITY SCALE (FORM D): A MEASURE 0F~A COMPONENT OF EMPATHY presented by , , Donald W. Werner ‘ has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for Ph.D. degree in ; Department of Counseling, Personnel ' Services and Educational Psychology Date 1///2/77 V 0-7639 @ I977 DONALD WILLIAM WERNER ALL RIGHTS RESERVED THE STRUCTURE, RELIABILITY AND VALIDITY OF THE AFFECTIVE SENSITIVITY SCALE (FORM D): A MEASURE OF A COMPONENT OF EMPATHY BY Donald W. Werner A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michgian State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of CounselingyPersonnel Services and Educational Psychology 1977 ABSTRACT THE STRUCTURE,RELIABILITY AND VALIDITY OF THE AFFECTIVE SENSITIVITY SCALE (FORM D): A MEASURE or? A COMPONENT OF EMPATHY BY Donald W. Werner The Affective Sensitivity Scale (A.S.S.) was first developed by Kagan and co-workers in 1962-65 as an instru- ment for the measurement of affective sensitivity. Affec- tive sensitivity was defined as the ability to accurately perceive and identify the feeling communication of another person. This behavior was believed to be a necessary com- ponent of empathy and its measurement useful in the devel- opment of counseling skills. This instrument, the A.S.S. was comprised of a sequence of videotaped scenes from actual counseling sessions and a multiple choice test requiring the identification of client feelings. Studies of the in- strument indicated that it was both a reliable and valid measure of affective sensitivity. A revised version of this instrument, the Affective Sensitivity Scale (Form D) has been created by Kagan and Schneider, which offers a much improved film portion and a new multiple choice test. This new version offers the ad- vantage of a wider range of stimulus situations, ranging from class room scenes to psychotherapy. The nature of Donald W. Werner the item alternatives was modified, as well as increased, in number and a revised scoring scheme provides subscale scoring which is specific to situation and setting. This study investigated the structure, reliability and validity of this new version of the Affective Sensitivity Scale. Normative data for the instrument has been provided based on 3103 subjects and norms are available for a number of educational and professional groups. The structure of the instrument was investigated by factor analysis which did not support the subscale scoring method although it did indicate responses to the instrument were situation specific. The instrument was reported to be reliable over a one week test/retest period (rho = 0.64) and the internal con- sistancy as measured by Chronbach's alpha was 0.75 for the total scale. The validity of this form of the A.S.S. (Form D) was investigated by testing several hypothesis. The results of the tests of these hypothesis are as follows: 1. The instrument is able to distinguish a high em— pathizer referent group (N = 18) from the norm group. 2. The effects of training known to be effective in increasing affective sensitivity can be distinguished by the instrument. 3. Members of homogeneous professional or educational groups have mean scores which are different from other dis— similar homogeneous groups of persons. Donald W. Werner 4. The instrument (A.S.S. - Form D) is not correlated with its predecessor (A.S.S. - Form C) and hence does not measure the same behaviors. 5. Scores on the new instrument are not correlated with being typed as a "thinker" or "feeler" on the Myers- Briggs Type Indicator, but is correlated with raw Extrover- sion and Judging scores. Based upon these results this form of the A.S.S. (Form D) has not been demonstrated to be a valid measure of affective sensitivity. The instrument would appear, how- ever, to be a reliable measure of some cognitive component of empathy. This conclusion was derived from all of the above findings plus an analysis of the items included in the multiple choice test portion of the instrument. The reporting of scale scores according to the nature of the stimulus situations (situation specific) appears to provide feedback to subjects which has been reported as offering considerable heuristic value. The instrument has a face validity which presently permits its use as a formative evaluative device, but which will require further develop- ment and modification if it is to be used as a summative evaluative instrument. To My Mother and In Memory of My Father ii ACKNOWLDEGMENTS This dissertation represents the results of the com- bined efforts of many persons, and while there is only one author, without the contributions of these other people it would never have been written. Norm Kagan, who served as my major professor was, and is a continuous source of inspiration, offering advice, support, and insight into what this work was all about. I feel a deep sense of appreciation for the personal involve- ment he has shared with me and his continued confidence in me both as a student and colleague. John Schneider, who as a committee member has had a significant impact on my thinking about this project, of- fered me insight into what the important questions were and shared a considerable part of the work of finding the answers. His influence on me has extended far beyond this work and has provided learnings which will continue to ' facilitate my own growth. Bruce Burke, who also willingly served as a committee member, reminded me that scholarly writing can also read well. His patient reading and editing of this thesis taught me about the logical progression of ideas and how to make the complicated seem almost simple. iii Bill Schmidt, who as a committee member shared his extensive understanding of statistical methodology, intro- duced a real sense of excitment about the capabilities of statistical procedures. He went beyond that which was re- quired by this limited study and his impact on this work will continue beyond its present stage of development. As a guidance committee these four individuals made this process into an enjoyable learning experience for me. At all times I was aware that they had confidence in me and the work I was doing. 'While I clearly began this process as a student I felt treated as a colleague, which certainly helped accelerate my own growth. Besides those directly involved with the writing of this dissertation a number of other persons contributed. First of all there are many unnamed persons who used the Affective Sensitivity Scale and shared their data with us. Without them we would be a long way behind. Ken Bullmer of Western Michigan University provided cross-validation measurements between various forms of the instrument. James Ayres of the University of Minnesota provided advice and suggestions on the development of the emperical scales. Thomas Parmeter of the Office of Medical Education, Research and Development (MSU) provided support in terms of computer time. A number of collegues in the Counseling Psychology program, at Lyman Briggs College and at STRIDE all made this venture feasible. They were excited by what I was iv doing, patient when my mind seemed elsewhere, and support- ive when the work was going slowly. I hope that they know in some small way the depth of appreciation that I feel for their friendships. Finally, this work was made easier because of the support of Susan (Hierta), who throughout the time this re- search was being undertaken, provided encouragement, caring and warmth. Her frequent insightful questions prevented many mistakes from being made. But most important she shared the joys of success as well as the frustrations of failure, and was a companion to talk things out with and a partner who shared in the enterprise. TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES O O O I O O O O O O O O 0 LIST OF FIGUMS O O O O O O O O O O O 0 CHAPTER I II INTRODUCTION . . . . . . . . . . Purpose . . . . . . . . . General Problem . . . . . Need for this Study . . . . . Definitions of Terms . . The Development of the Origina The Creation of a New Revised Instrument . . . . . . . Research Questions . . . . Delimitations of this Study General Outline of the Study REVIEW OF THE RELATED LITERATURE Definitions of Empathy . . . . . Empathy as Cognition . . . . . . Empathy as Affect and Cognition . Empathy as Affect . . . . . . . . The Measurement of Empathy . . . Predictive Measures of Empathy . Situational Measures of Empathy . Instruments Which Employ Photographs and Pictures as Stimulus . . Measures Which Employ Written Client Statements 0 O O O O O O O 0 Measures Which Employed Audio Recordings as Stimuli . . . . . . . . . a1 Scale Ve ersion Measures Which Employ Video Media . Measures that Employ Live Situations for the Measurement of Empathy . Other Measures of Empathy . . Empathy in the Psychological Literature . smary O I O O O O O O O O O O 0 vi t O I O O D. C O O O O Page ix OIU'INlr-‘H l-‘ 14 14 16 17 17 19 23 27 33 34 35 38 42 47 51 S3 58 CHAPTER III IV STUDY METHODOLOGY AND PROCEDURES . . . Introduction . . . . . .'. . . . . . Sample Population and Computer Scoring Optimization of Scoring . . . . . . . Research Questions and Hypothesis . . Research Procedures . . . . . . . . . 'The Structure of the Scale-Factor Analysis . . . . . . . . . . . . Reliability Studies . . . . . . Questions of Validity . . . . . Summary . . . . . . . . . . . . . . ANALYSIS OF RESULTS . . . . . . . . . Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . Hypothesis Related to the Structure of Instrument . . . . . . . . . . . . . Reliability of the Instrument . . . . Validity of the A.S.S. . . . . . . . . Summary of Findings . . . . . . . . . DISCUSSION AND CONCLUSIONS . . . . . . Summary . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Structure of the Instrument . . . Reliability of the Instrument . Validity of the Instrument . . . Conclusions . . . . . . . . . . . . Discussion . . . . . . . . . . . . . Comparison of Form C and Form D . . . Present Utility of the Affective Sensitiv- ity Scale (Form D) . . . . . . . . . Implications for Further Research on the Present Form of the Instrument . . . Implications for Modification of A.S.S. (FormD).............. APPENDIX C O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O A B :20”! MUG Affective Sensitivity Scale, Form C . Affective Sensitivity Scale: Examiner' Manual, Form D and E . . . . . . . . Scales and Subscales, Item Content . . Affective Sensitivity Scale, Form D . Norms for Affective Sensitivity Scale, Form D . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Inter-Item Correlations . . . . . . . Inter-Scale Correlations . . . . . Summary of the Overlap of Factors and Subscales . . . . . . . . . . . vii Page 62 62 63 71 74 74 75 77 82 83 83 83 88 93 106 109 109 114 115 116 119 122 123 127 128 131 136 136 151 167 173 215 228 231 232 Page I Computer Produced Scoring . . . . . . . . . . . 233 BIBLIOGRAPHY O O O O O I O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 2 34 viii 10. 11. 12. 13. 14. 15. LIST OF TABLES Distribution of Preferred, Acceptable, and Other Item Choices for the Four Revisions . Item Analysis of A.S.S. (Form D) Revision Four 0 O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 0 Summary of Item Analysis Data for Form D . Distribution of the 3103 Subjects, by Educa- tion or Profession, Used to Establish Norm- ative Data for the A.S.S. (Form D) . . . . Results of Principal Component Factor Anal- ysis: Eigenvalues and Percent of Variances Results of Principal Component Factor Anal- ysis: Factors and Item Loadings . . . . . Summary of Item Loading on First Factor . . Results of Test-Retest Study for A.S.S. (Form D) - One Week Interval . . . . . . . Internal Consistancy of A.S.S. (Form D): Chronbach's Alpha . . . . . . . . . . . . . Comparison of High Empathizer and Low Empa- thizer Referent Groups with Norm Group . . Data Resulting from the Administration of A.S.S. (Form D) on a Pretest and Posttest Basis to Groups Involved in Communication Skill WOrkshops . . . . . . . . . . . . . . ANOVA and Post Hoc Test for Comparable Groups 0 O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 0 Correlations Between A.S.S. Form C and A.S.S. Fom D O O O O O O O I O O O O O O 0 Correlation Between MBTI and A.S.S. (Form D) by Typology . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Correlation Between MBTI and A.S.S. (Form D) by Raw Scores . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . ix Page 67 68 70 71 84 86 87 89 92 94 97 100 103 105 107 LIST OF FIGURES Figure Page I Homogeneous Groups (95% Confidence Interval for Mean Total Scores) . . . . . . . . . . . . 101 CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION Purpose The general purpose of this study was to investigate the structure, validity, and reliability of a new version of the Affective Sensitivity Scale. The instrument was developed as a measure of affective sensitivity, a significant com- ponent of empathy. Previous versions of this instrument have been reported as valid and reliable. This study sought to de- termine if the new instrument measures what the earlier ver- sions measured. Further, the study explored what modif- ications may be needed to increase the scale's utility. ‘General Problem Empathy has been described as a significant factor in the processes of counseling and psychotherapy. It has also been postulated as a central factor in nearly all interper- sonal communication; specifically to the extent that the presence or absence of empathy is significant in predicting the outcome of many kinds of human interaction. Positive outcome of therapy has been found by a number of studies to be correlated with the therapist's empathic abilities. The study of empathy is considered to be an important 2 area of research in the psychological and social sciences. For such studies to be possible, it is necessary that re- searchers share a common definition of the term which is sufficiently specific to distinguish it from other related terms. In addition, there need exist instruments which can measure the degree to which the "empathy" is or is not present in the subgect. Need for this Study While there is significant agreement on the importance of empathy in interpersonal communication, there is not agreement on the definition of empathy. Several different definitions exist and what is understood to represent empa- thy in one study is not considered to be empathy in another. In addition, tests which have been developed to measure em- pathy measure different qualities. One way to avoid the confusion inherent in the use of the term empathy is to use terms which are less global and more descriptive of measurable behavior. While empathy may be seen as the "imaginative projection of one's own conscious- ness into another being" or the "putting oneself in the place of another," it necessarily requires the ability to identify the affective state of another. The term affective sensi- tivity represents the ability of a person to accurately de- code (detect and identify) the affective communication of another person. Unlike the term "empathy," the term 3 "affective sensitivity" is more easily operationalized and the development of tests to measure its presence in inter- personal communication is possible. Hence the use of the more limited concept of affective sensitivity may permit more meaningful discriminations to be made. For example, it is difficult to speak of a person as demonstrating a high level of empathy if researchers apply different definitions to the term. There would likely be more agreement if the subjects were described in such terms as demonstrating a high degree of accuracy in decoding the affective messages of other persons. While affective sensi- tivity is not empathy, as mentioned previously, it is likely to be a significant part of being empathic. An instrument for the measurement of affective sen- sitivity was developed by Kagan and his co-workers in 1962- 1965. The development of this instrument, the Affective Sensitivity Scale, provided in part what was lacking in the empathy measures available at that time. The instru- ment was unique in that it avoided the ambiguities in the definition of empathy while at the same time, measured be- haviors which are likely to be included in most any defin- ition of empathy. In addition, it measured affective sen- sitivity under circumstances which approximated real-life by providing both auditory and visual stimuli. This was accomplished by employing a sequence of video tape segments of counseling sessions and a set of objective questions a- bout the feelings portrayed in these segments. This result- ed in an instrument which could be economically administered to large numbers of people and which was easy to score. Subsequent research on the instrument demonstrated it to be a valid and reliable measure of affective sensitivity. During the last few years, it has become apparent that there are weaknesses in the instrument. Its reliance on video taped stimulus means that the persons shown during the presentation of the scale are dressed in the fashions of fifteen years ago and many of the concerns expressed by these people are dated. In addition, advances in the state- of-the-art of video media has improved considerably so that the instrument is of technically poor quality when compared to that which is available today. In response to these deficiencies and in an attempt to provide a measure of affective sensitivity which would have greater discriminating powers, a new version of the instru- ment has been developed by Kagan and Schneider (1975). This new Affective Sensitivity Scale exists in two forms (known as Form D and Form B) which, while modeled after their earlier version, has been modified both in content and in scoring. This new instrument has been well received by the pro- fessional community as is evidenced by the more than fifty users and the greater than four thousand administrations that have been completed to date. Yet for use in research, this instrument also needs to be demonstrated as reliable and valid as a measure of affective sensitivity. The purpose of this studywas therefore to meet the above 5 stated need. This involved investigating the extent to which this new version of the Affective Sensitivity Scale measures the same behaviors as the earlier scale and to de- termine the degree to which it is both reliable and valid as a measure of affective sensitivity. Definitions of Terms Affective Sensitivity is defined as "the ability to detect and describe the immediate affective state of an- other, or in terms of communication theory, the ability to receive and decode affective communication" (Kagan, Krath- wohl, and Farquhar, 1965). For purposes of this study, def- initions of empathy will be discussed; but in view of the semantic confusion and theoretical disagreements which are associated with the term, no new overarching definition will be offered. The measurement of affective sensitivity, a more limited construct, is the primary interest of this study. Affective Sensitivity Scale - refers to both the film portion and the associated multiple choice test of the instrument. The initials A.S.S. will be used as an abbreviation. Since there are other versions of this instrument in existence, unless otherwise noted the term Affective Sensitivity Scale or A.S.S. will refer to Form D. In addition, the scoring key for this form has under- gone several revisions, the last being Revision Four. Un- less otherwise noted all scoring referred to in this report will refer to this Revision Four Scoring. The film which is the stimulus part of the instru- ment is comprised of a series of short film segments which will be referred to as scenes. A sequence of scenes which include the same people will be referred to as an episode. The instrument will refer to the affective sensitiv- ity film and the multiple choice test which accompanies it. The Development of the Original Scale The Affective Sensitivity Scale was created between 1962-1965 by Kagan and his co-workers (Chapman, 1966) to provide a reliable and valid measure of affective sensitiv- ity. This instrument was specifically designed for use in evaluating the affective sensitivity of counselors. The instrument included a series of video-taped segments of actual counseling sessions, most of which involved high school aged clients. The subject watched and listened to a short segment of the session, usually about a minute or two in duration, and then responded to several multiple choice questions about the client's feelings about them- selves (self-awareness) or about the client's perception of the relationship between him or her and the counselor. The subjects' responses were scored as either correct or incor- rect and a single scale score was produced by counting the number of correct responses. The instrument underwent several revisions in the course of its development and was extensively investigated 7 with regards to its validity and reliability. Concurrent validity was indicated by a 0.53 Correlation between coun- selor ratings of affective sensitivity in M.A. counselor training groups and the instrument (Kagan, pp 31., 1967,“ p. 177). In a study which compared subjective supervisor ratings of NDEA students and A.S.S. scores, a correlation of 0.42 to 0.16 was reported, while peer ratings of counselor sensitivity had a correlation of 0.64 to -0.10 with the A.S.S. A predictive validity study by Kagan, pt 31., (1967) reported a correlation of 0.49 between initial A.S.S. scores and peer ratings of counselor effectiveness for students in- volved in a year long NDEA institute. In general, the A.S.S. was noted to be a better predictor of failure as a counselor than of success. Kuder-Richardson split-half reliability for the scale was reported to range from 0.53 to 0.77 for homogeneous groups of counselors or counselor trainees, with the major- ity of the values above 0.70 (Kagan, 1967, p. 189). Most of the original research was carried out employ- ing a version of the instrument which contained 89 miltiple choice items and which presented 41 videotaped segments of 11 different clients and counselors. This was referred to as revised Form.B (Campbell, 1967,). Subsequently, a shorter version (Form C) was created from the revised Form B by'elim- inating 22 items and 8 videotaped segments. Kagan (1977) reported that item analysis indicated that this shorter version provided the same relative scores as the previous form. Most of the research since 1971 has employed this newer (Form C) version (Archer, 1971; Dendy, 1971; Rowe, 1972). A copy of the multiple choice portion of this in- strument can be found in Appendix A. In the last few years it has become clear that there are several serious limitations to this version of the scale. The most apparent weakness is in the audio and visual qual- ities of the videotaped portion of the instrument. While its video and sound qualities represented the quality that was then state-of-the-art, it is poor when compared to what is available today. It is known that poor sound quality re- sults in significantly lower performance scores (Campbell, 1968, p. 81). Not only was the sound quality poor when the instrument was created, but subsequent copies and recopies have deteriorated. It has been the experience of some re- searchers that frustration with the at times almost inaudible sound has resulted in serious biasing of scale results for some individuals. It is likely that the instru- ment discriminates against those with some form of hearing loss. A second limitation is the rather limited range of situations presented in the video excerpts. These included settings which were exclusively counseling and the majority of the clients were high school students. This meant that the content of the instrument was less familiar to persons in professions other than counseling, such as medical students, 9 teachers, etc. Other limitations included the dating of the video sequences in terms of dress and commonality of problems presented, for while most of the concerns are still relevant today, the norms of persons in this age group have changed. The Creation of a New Revised Version of the Instrument A new version of the instrument has been created which is intended to correct some of the faults of the previous version. It offers the advantages of a more refined scoring system and a more fully developed set of normative data. While the earlier versions were black and white, the new form is in color and was produced under more ideal studio conditions. It uses film rather than videotape to produce a scale of much greater fidelity and to avoid the variations inherent in videotape playback equipment which is often in improper working conditions. The filmed sequences include a much wider range of situations, persons, ages, and emotions. Not only are counseling situations presented, but the new version provides situations from medical, psychotherapeu- tic, informal, group, and educational settings. In addition the new film invites the subject to focus on the affect of all the persons involved in the scene, while the earlier version referred only to the affect of the client. Besides the changes in the film portion, there have been changes in the multiple choice response portion of the test. This includes the existence of five multiple choice responses instead of three, which includes both affective and 10 cognitive statements, plus a significant revision of the scoring procedure. The revised Form C had 66 items which yielded a single total score based on a correct (1 point) versus incorrect (0 point) item choice. The new form has 63 items, but these are scored as preferred (2 points), acceptable (1 point), or random (0 point). In addition to a single total score, the scale offers subscoring—-the sub- scores representing subsets of items which have in common a single sex, level of maturation, or situation. So all items which involve males are scored together, as are the items in the following dichotomies: client/interviewer; adult/ child; male/female; group/dyad; education/healthcare/infor- mal/counseling/ and psychotherapy setting. A description of these scales can be found in the users manual which is in- cluded in Appendix B. The reason for these subscores is that the instrument's authors postulated that a person's affective sensitivity was likely situation dependent and that issues of age, sex, status, setting, and the numbers of individuals present in a situation would influence that person's affective sensitivity. In addition to the above subscale scores, nine empir- ical subscales or indicies were created to see if there were any other underlying structure to the instrument. These em- pirical scales were defined as follows: High Intensity (High): A subscale composed of 31 items which referred to scenes in which the emotions ex— pressed were intense. 11 Impactful (Impact): A subscale composed of 26 items which referred to scenes in which the content was impactful, such as talking about death, but in which the emotions ex- pressed were not strong. Inferred (Interp): A subscale composed of 25 items which referred to persons who were frequently not visible to the subject or who provided few cues as to their emotions. The subject had to infer the affect by reference to the sit- uation presented or by the responses of other persons in the film. Observed (Observ): A subscale which referred to items in which affect could be clearly observed. Involved 32 items. Suspiciousness (Susp): An empirical index which counted the number of incorrect item choices which implied suspic- iousness on the part of the subject toward the person re- ferred to in the item choice. It included a total of 31 possible choices which came from 30 items. Discounting (Disct): An empirical index which recorded the number of incorrect responses chosen by the subject which implied that the person on film referred to was not capable of dealing with their own feelings, nor capable of taking care of themselves. A total of 48 choices were possible from 42 items. Pollyanna (Poly): An empirical index which recorded the number of incorrect choices chosen by the subject which implied that the situation presented was more positive or 12 problem free than it actually was. Included only six item choices. Feeling Denial (Feeldn): An empirical index based on incorrect item choices which denied the expressed feelings of the person referred to by the item. Thirteen item choices made up the index. Feeling Awareness (Feelar): An empirical index which was produced from correct item choices which indicated ack- nowledgement of feeling self-awareness expressed by the per- son referred to by the item. A total of 20 item choices comprised this index. A table of the items or item choices which made up the empirical subscalescnrindicies can be found in Appendix C. These nine empirical subscales or indicies are not con- sidered as a formal part of the instrument and are not de- scribed in the users manual, although they have been pro- vided to the instrument's users in order to encourage discussion. An example of the reporting format may be found in Appendix I. Not only was the new version of the Affective Sensitiv— ity Scale (A.S.S.) different in both the film and written portions, it was provided in two different forms, each of 63 items length and with similar but not identical film portions. These were labeled as Form D and Form E. At this time only the form D is in wide use with the latter form awaiting validation of the former. The existence of two forms of the scale potentialy permits using different 13 forms of the instrument for pre and post testing. A copy of the multiple choice test manual for Form D can be found in Appendix D. Research Questions As mentioned previously, for this instrument to be a viable tool in research, it must be demonstrated as both reliable and valid. In addition the nature of the instru- ment itself needs to be better understood. For example, do all the items included in the test discriminate positively between high and low scores? The questions raised by the authors when they proposed the subscale scoring need to be considered, namely is affective sensitivity a unitary factor or is it situation specific? There are three general research questions to which this study addressed itself. They are as follows: 1. Is the affective Sensitivity construct a simple construct as is suggested by the single scale scoring of the earlier forms or is it more situa- tion specific as is suggested by the new versions subscale scoring? ‘ 2. Is the new A.S.S. a reliable measure of affective sensitivity? 3. Is the new A.S.S. a valid measure of affective sensitivity and if so, does it measure the same behaviors as did the earlier scale? This study will address these questions only to the newly 14 created Form D of the instrument. Delimitations of this Study This study is intended to be a preliminary investiga- tion of the new version of the Affective Sensitivity Scale (A.S.S.) Form D. This study will determine if the new in- strument measures the same behaviors as the previous scale, but if what it measures is different no steps will be taken to determine what it measures, although possible explana- tions will be offered. While a number of the specific procedures employed in this study will involve statistical inferences the focus will be on meaningfulness, not simply significance. The primary goal is to understand how well this instrument operates as a measure of affective sensitivity and to gain some insight into what the scores mean when considering the potential of the instrument as a research tool. General Outline of the Study This study began by developing methods for computer scoring of the instrument. Normative data was collected and item analysis is employed to optomize the scoring procedures. Once a scoring procedure is decided the study proceeds to consider the three research questions, namely to investi- gate the structure, validity, and reliabilty of the instru- ment. A review of the literature related to empathy and its measurement appears in Chapter II. The experimental 15 methodology is presented in Chapter III along with a des- cription of the populations used in the study, the experi- mental design and methods of analysis, and a statement of the specific research questions. Chapter IV contains the results of the various analysis employed. Chapter V con- tains a summary, conclusions, discussion, and suggestions for future research. CHAPTER II REVIEW OF THE RELATED LITERATURE Empathy is frequently seen as a critical parameter in a wide range of interpersonal processes, from psychotherapy to marital harmony. This review will consider the liter- ature on empathy from three perspectives: (l) the ways in which it is defined, (2) the methodologies of measurement, and (3) the research on the significance of empathy in in- terpersonal situations. Empathy can be conceptualized as the ability to place oneself in the role of another person. It represents the ability to comprehend events as another person sees them, to appreciate them as though you were that person. While this definition would appear to have general agreement among most writers, there is not common agreement on what indicates the presence of empathy in a person. Recent reviews on em- pathy (Gladstein, 1977; Iannitti, 1975) have catagorized the indicators of empathy as either cognitive, affective-cogni- tive or affective. So either a person needs to have a cog- nitive understanding of another to be empathic in some definitions, cognitive understanding mediated by an affective response in others, or simply an affective response to an- other in still other definitions. In addition to these 16 l7 categorical definitions each type of definition has associ- ated with it one or more operational definitions. These latter definitions are dependent on some sort of instrument or scale to provide a measurable quantity which is supposed to be synonomous with the categorical definition. This review will employ this three level approach in presenting the various ways in which the term empathy is employed. The operation definitions will be presented more fully in the section on measurement. Definitions of Empathy Empathy as ngnition Hogan (1975) suggests that an empathic person is one who "is socially perceptive of a wide range of interpersonal areas . . . (and) seems to be aware of the impression he makes on others" (p. 15). Hence to be empathic is to know the impact that one makes on others, to know how they are likely to respond to you and to know what is their likely reactions, opinions, feelings, etc. Other authors concurs with this definition. Weinstein (1969) asserts that "the individual must be able to take the role of the other ac- curately; he must be able to correctly predict the impact that various lines of action will have on other's definition of the situation" (p. 757). Others (Aronfreed, 1969; Burns and Cavey, 1959; Dymond, 1949; Feshback and Roe,' 1968; Shelton, 1949) support the notion that the abil- ity to place oneself in the role 18 of another, is a central factor in being empathic; This definition of empathy is easily operationalized and a num- ber of instruments have been developed to measure empathy defined in this manner (Dymond, 1949, 1950; Hogan, 1969; Kerr, 1947). A number of writers contend however that this defin- ition is too limited. They maintain that the ability to identify with others or the ability to accurately predict the behavior of others does not entirely describe the pro- cess. Feshback and Feshback (1969) point out that "social insight or the ability to understand and predict another person's feeling and behavior" is insufficient for acting empathically. Feshback and Roe (1968) point out that "this approach conceives of empathy as primarily a cognitive phen- omenon and fails to consider the affective connotation of empathy as a vicarious emotional response of a perceiver to the emotional experience of a perdeived objectf' According to Iannotti "individuals who are high in em- pathy are those who frequently and appropriately respond to the feelings of others." Hence a cognitive definition of empathy limits the definition in such a way as not to distin- guish those persons who have social insight but who fail to respond_appropriately to others to indicate that they (1) un- derstand and (2) are concerned about the feelings of others. Shantz (1975) supports this notion by indicating that cogni- tive understanding and affective responsiveness are not al- ways found together. l9 Empathy as Affect and Cognition Certainly the most commonly employed definitions of empathy used today are those in which both cognition and affect play an integral role. The inclusion of both these processes allows a distinction to be made between empathy and other similar but not identical behaviors, such as iden- tification, projection, and sympathy. There is, as Fash- back (1975) points out a certain confusion as to the rela- tionship between the affect and the cognition, for "it is possible to conceptualize empathy as a cognitive product mediated by emotional factors or as an affective response mediated by cognitive processes" (p. 25). But while this distinction may be important in some cases in instances it is more useful to understand the process as an interaction of cognition and affect. For example Carl Rogers, defines empathy as: The way of being with another person which is termed empathic has several facets. It means enter- ing the private perceptual world of the other and becoming thoroughly at home in it. It involved being sensitive, moment to moment, to the changing felt meanings which flow in this other person, to the fear or rage or tenderness or confusion or whatever, that he/she is experiencing. It means temporarily living in his/her life, moving about in it delicately without making judgments, sensing meanings of which he/she is scarcely aware, but not trying to uncover feelings of which the person is totally unaware, since this would be too threatening. It includes communicating your sensings of his/her world as you look with fresh and unfrightened eyes at elements of which the individual is fearful. It means frequently checking with him/her as to the accuracy of your sensings, and being guided by the responses you receive. You are a confident com- panion to the person in his/her inner world. By point- ing to the possible meanings in the flow of his/her experiencing you help the person to focus on this 20 useful type of referent, to experience the meanings more fully, and to move forward in the experiencing. To be with another in this way means that for the time being you lay aside the views and values you hold for yourself in order to enter another's world preju- dice. In some sense it means that you lay aside your self and this can only be done by a person who is secure enough in himself that he knows he will not get lost in what may turn out to be the strange or bizarre world of the other, and can comfortably return to his. own world when he wishes. Perhaps this description makes clear that being empathic is a complex, demanding, strong yet subtle and gentle way of being. Rogers (1975, p. 4) While Rogers' definition is far from operational or testable it indicates that being empathic is both a cognitive and affective process in that it involves comprehending the "meanings".H m.~ 6.~ n.6 «moansoflowdnsm 6: 6H.o m.ou p.m 6.6~ Ho. a>.~ ~.6 6.n 6.~m «.6 h.6~ newu8>ummno a: o~.~ 6.6- ~.6 6.5m Ho. 66.~ ~.m o.6 6.6m 5.6 «.mm 00cmumucu 6: 66.o o.Ha m.~ a.m~ Ho. 6m.~ n.n 6.m «.om 6.6 a.6~ ”guacamEH 6: ~6.H «.6- H.m m.6~ H6. ac.n 5.6 6.6 6.5m 6.6 H.~m suwmcmucH no“: 6: o6.H 6.6- 6.6 H.O6 Ho. hm.~ ~.m 6.6 p.6n H.na 6.56 Hmuoa 6: mm.o 6.H- m.m 6.6a Ho. HH.m m.m 6.6 m.m~ m.m o.o~ mauuonuoeoxma 6: od.o H.o+ 6.~ H.HH m: H6.H ~.H m.H ~.~H 6.~ o.HH mafiaouqsoo 6: 66.H 6.Hu v.fi m.6 ma 66.H m.a 6.~ «.6 a.~ 6.6 Husuouca an 66.6 m.H- m.~ v.m m: oH.H o.H ~.m n.HH 6.m s.oH zuammz m: cm.” 6.~u o.m c.6a m: ~6.H m.H 0.6 a.o~ v.6 6.6H coauoosvm Ho. «6.6 6.6- 6.6. 6.66 Ho. 65.~ ”.6 a.» «.66 w.oa 6.mm case 6: m~.o 6.6- o.~ m.m~ m: 6~.~ H.~ 6.~ 6.6H m.m ~.ma daouo a: wh.d o.ml N.m o.mN Ho. mm.~ v.n o.m «.mm H.w o.mN Geneva 6: FH.H 6.m- m.6 H.6n Ho. 66.~ 6.6 6.6 6.66 6.6 n.6n was: 6: 6m.H 6.Hu H.~ 6.6 6: o6.H H.H p.~ ~.HH m.~ H.6H cease m: HQ.H m.mn m.m o.am Hc. mm.N H.m m.h m.mm o.HH v.5m Hanna 6: nn.o p.au 6.6 6.6m 6: mv.~ 6.n o.6 o.Hm «.6 n.6u um:o«>u0u:H a: vh.d O.VI m.m m.m~ ma vH.N m.m m.v m.~m m.w m.mN acuuau vm u 6 am m .vm u 6 am m cm m madam msouu nouanummem 30a moouu nouacuoqemucmwz msouw Euoz An Buomv mamom xuw>wuwucom o>wuoouu< HOu msouo Euoz nua3 mmsouo ucmuowom nouwnummem 30A can HenwcuomEm no“: no comfiuomeou .oa manna 95 test such as Hotelling's T2. Nevertheless it may be conclu- ded that the high empathizer referent group can be distin- guished from the norm group on Total scale scores and on most of the subscales and emperical scales. The magnitude of the difference ranges between .5 to .7 standard deviation for the majority of the scales. Hence, while the referent group does have higher scores, they are not very much higher. Since part of this referent group was employed in the first revision of the scoring for this instrument, the re- sults are likely confounded. Yet while this is the case, the subsequent three revisions removed the gains made for this group on the first revision. The differences which exist at present are therefore not present simply because the instrument was revised to obtain them. Hypothesis VI: A low empathizer referent group will score lower than the norm group on all scales of the A.S.S. (Form D) As with the previous hypothesis this was tested by means of a single t-test. A low empathizer referent group (N = 9) was obtained in part from a group of student tea- chers for whom behavioral ratings, which included sensitiv- ity to feelings, were available. The findings and statist- ical results are summarized in Table 10. No significant differences can be observed between the referent and norm group on any of the subscales and differ- ences are found for only two of the nine empirical scales. 96 However the mean scores are about a half standard deviation below the norm group. Hence it may not be concluded that the A.S.S. was able to distinguish between.a low empathizer referent group and the general population. Hypothesis VII: The post test scores of persons in programs demonstrated to be effective in increasing affective sensitivity will be higher than their pre- test scores. This hypothesis was tested by using paired t-tests on subjects scores obtained both before and after a training program. A total of 371 persons involved in fourteen dif- ferent programs were tested. The results of this analysis can be found in Table 11. The primary mode of training in- volved is IPR (Interpersonal Process Recall) although some of the programs included listening skill training, interview- ing, and HRI training. Significant positive gain scores were observed for nine of the fourteen groups with gain scores ranging from 5 to 14 points, with the mean gain score for the entire population being 4.8, and for the IPR train- ing groups 5.3. None of the groups indicated a decrease in scores. Since this population was employed as a basis for scor- ing Revision Four, they are necessarily confounded. Never- theless, these differences existed prior to the revision, although smaller in magnitude. Subsequent to this revision, therefore, two additional groups were considered. While no positive gain scores were observed, this particular population failed to demonstrate positive gain for one of 97 66. 66.6 66.66 66.66 66.66 66.66 666 66666666 6666666 666o66666666 66 666x666 66. 66.6 66.66 66.66 66.66 66.66 666 66666666 6666666 666666666600 66 666\66 6666. 66.6 66.66 66.66 66.66 66.66 666 66666666 666 66666600 666 6666. 66.6 66.66 66.66 66.66 66.66 666 666066 66666606 666 66. 66.6 6.6 66.66 6.6 66.66 666 66666666 6oo666 6666 66 66x66 666. 66.6 6.6 66.66 6.66 66.66 666 6666666666 6666666666 66 66x66 666. 66.6 6.66 66.66 6.66 66.66 666660 666366>66666 66666666 666666: 66 66\66 66. 66.- 6.66 66.66 6.66 66.66 666666666 6666666600 6oo666 66 66\66.6 66. 66.6 6.66 66.66 6.66 66.66 666 6 666 66666666666666 66 66\66 666. 66.6 6.66 66.66 6.66 66.66 666 66666666666666 66 66\66 .666. 66.6 6.66 66.66 6.66 66.66 66666 666666666 66660666666666666 666666 66666: 66 66\66 666. 66.6 6.6 66.66 6.66 66.66 666 66666>66666 66606666666 66 66\66 66. 66.6 6.6 66.66 6.66 66.66 666 66666>66666 66606666666 66 6\6 .666. 66.66 6.66 66.66 6.66 66.66 666 66666666 6666666 666066666600 66 66x66 66. 66.- 6.66 66.66 6.66 66.66 666 66666666 6666666 666066666666 66 66x66 6666. 66.66 6.66 66.66 6.66 66.66 666 6666666666066 6660 666666 66 66\66 6666. 66.66 6.66 66.66 6.6 66.66 666 6666066666066 6660 666666 66 66x66 666. 6.6 6.66 66.66 6.66 66.66 666 6666066666066 6660 666666 66 6\6 006606666666 066650 cm 6606 m 6606 cm 066 k 066 66666666 6066666606 .2 65060 umonuxuox 666xm 6066606656600 :6 00>6o>66 669060 06 a6n6n 66066606 666 6666666 6 so 69 5606. .w.m.< mo :o6uouum6cweod may 2066 666669606 0660 .66 o6n66 98 its two previous groups. Hence the result was predictable for this population and therefore we may still reasonably conclude that the A.S.S. is able to distinguish the effects of training. Hypothesis VIII: Persons who are members of groups known to be homogeneous with regard to education or profession will have total scale scores on the A.S.S. (Form D) which are more similar to their own referent group than to any other dissimilar group of persons. This hypothesis was tested by comparing the group means of seven sample populations which account for two-thirds of the subjects involved in this study. This included under- graduates, undergraduate social work students, occupational therapy trainees, first year medical students, family prac- tice residents, psychiatry residents, and corrections workers. A one-way ANOVA was computed for these seven pop- ulations (see Table 12) and since this was significant, a post-hoc test performed to c0mpare the means. The results of the one-way analysis indicated that the means of the seven groups were not identical. The 95% confidence inter- val for these means has been represented graphically in Figure 1. The plot of the group means and confidence intervals supports the contention that the group means are dissimilar. The post-hoc test, the LSD (least-significant difference test), indicated the majority of the means were different, although adjacent means did overlap. 99 A second way of looking at the relationship between these populations is to compare the variance between the groups to the variance within the groups. If the group scores are very different from.one another than the vari- ance within groups would be smaller than between groups. This can be determined by calculating.the interclass cor- relation coefficient (R). This coefficient is determined by employing the formula MSB'MSW MSB + (K-l) MS R: W where K is the average size of all the populations. The interclass correlation coefficient (R) for these groups is therefore 0.037, which means that only 4% of the variance may be attributed as between groups while 96% is within groups. This result would indicate that while the mean scores are different for different educational or pro- fessional populations there is considerable variability of scores within these populations. Since the mean scores do differ significantly this would suggest that, while there is considerable variance within groups, some populations are more likely to have a dispropionate number of high scores while other populations have a dispropionate number of lower scores. With some certainty it can be concluded that groups of persons will more likely have total scores on the A.S.S. 100 Table 12. ANOVA and Post Hoc Test for Comparable Groups-- Affective Sensitivity Scale N = 1962 Source df 33 ms f p less than Between 6 11,496 1916 11.88 .0001 Within 1955 315,328 161.3 Total 1961 326,823 Group* Count Mean SD 95% Confidence Interval A 408 68.4 12.6 67.2 to 69.6 B 426 67.1 12.0 65.9 to 69.6 C 58 60.1 12.6 56.8 to 68.2 D 24 56.2 15.9 49.4 to 62.9 E 837 69.6 13.0 68.7 to 70.5 F 149 70.9 12.7 68.8 to 72.9 G 60 64.2 12.4 60.9 to 67.4 Total 1962 68.3 12.9 67.7 to 68.9 Multiple Range Test--LSD Procedure for the .05 Level Homogeneous Subsets Subset 1 2 ca U? h w- 1._. *See Figure 2 for a description Groups C & D C & G B & G A & B A & E F E & J— of these populations. 101 70" H (U > H 3 a 65— H 0) U C Q) '0 -H m: G 8 60" 69 m ox m 0) H O U U) G 4 m 55 0) Z 50“ v —-—4 r—m—a i———*r.1-—-l r—m——I Figure 1. Key: Group Homogeneous Groups (95% confidence interval for mean total scores). ommonmzv Medical Students N = 408 Occupational Therapists N = Family Practice Residents N Psychiatry Residents N = 24 Undergraduate Students N = 837 Undergraduate Social Work Students N = 149 Corrections Workers N = 60 426 = 58 102 which are more likely to similar to persons of the same educational level or profession than to persons of different education or profession. Hypothesis IX: Scores on the A.S.S. (Form D) will have a high positive correlation with scores on the earlier (Form C) version of the instrument. This hypothesis was tested by computing Pearson cor- relations for 113 subjects who had taken both forms of the scale. These correlations are tabulated in Table 13. The correlations between the two forms were uniformly negative and the magnitude of the correlations were low, usually less than 0.2. In particular, the total scale score was correlated -.15, although this was a non-significant correl- ation (P < 0.12). Since Form C has two distinct kinds of items, i.e., items which refer to the clients self-awareness, and to his preception of his relationship with the counselor, sub- scores for these items were computed and also correlated with the A.S.S. The correlations with the self-awareness items were generally smaller and less negative than with the relationship items. Im may therefore be concluded that scores on the A.S.S. (Form D) do not have a high positive correlation with the earlier (Form C) A.S.S. In addition, those correlations which do exist are negative. The A.S.S. (Form D) is more highly (although negative) correlated with relationship items on Form C than with self-awareness items. .103 Table 13. Correlation Between A.S.S. Form C and A.S.S. Form D N a 113 Form C Form C Form C Scale Total Score Self-Awareness Relationship Rh£) Pst RhCD Pummno «H. «H.- ««. «o. «6. Ho.- o«. HH.- «H. «H.- HH. «H. oo. ««.- «o. ««. aocmumocH «H. «H.- «o. «H. H«. oH.- «H. «H. ««. «o.- ««. oH. «o. H«.- «o. ««. HsouomasH «o. oH.- «o. H«. «6. Ho.- o«. «oo. oo. «o. oo. «o. «o. «H.- «o. «H. «uHmowuoH o«Hm «o. «H.- oo. «H. «o. «o.- H«. «o.- ««. «o.- o«. «o. Ho. o«.- «o. o«. Hmuoe «H. «H.- «H. «H. «o. «o.- ««. «o. ««. oo.- ««. «o. oo. ««.- Ho. o«. «muuonuoooxmo o«. HH.- «H. «H. «H. «H.- ««. oH. «o. «o.- ««. «o. «o. oH.- oo. ««. «:HHmmcooo «o. o«.- «o. ««. ««. «o.- «o. «o.- ««. «o. o«. oo.- «o. o«.- «H. «H. Huguoon ««. oH.- ««. oo.- «H. HH.- HH. «H.- H«. oH.- «H. «H. «o. o«.- oH. «H. :uHomx oH. «H.- «o. «H. oo. «H. «o. «H.- «o. «o.- ««. «o. «H. «H.- «H. «H. ooHuuooom oo. ««.- «o. H«. o«. «o.- «6. Ho. Ho. «o.- H«. «o. Ho. H«.- Ho. ««. vase o«. «o.- ««. «o. «H. «H. «o. ««.- ««. «o.- «H. «H. ««. oH.- «H. 6H” macho «o. «H.- oH. «H. «o. ooo.- oH. oH.- o«. «o. «o. «o.- «o. ««.- «o. o«. oHusmm «H. «H.- «H. «H. H«. «o.- «6. Ho. «H. «H.- «H. HH. «o. ««.- oo. ««. -oHoz «o. «H. «o. o«. oo. «H. ««. oH.- o«. «o.- ««. «o. ««. «o.- «o. Ho. oHHoo oH. «H.- oH. «H. o«. «o.- o«. «o.- o«. «o.- o«. «o. Ho. ««.- Ho. o«. uHoo< «H. HH.- o«. «o. o«. oo.- ««. «o.- «o. «o.- ««. «o. «o. ««.- Ho. ««. 6636H>uouoH «o. H«.- «o. o«. «o. «o. «6. Ho.- o«. «o.- ««. oH. oo. ««.- oo. ««. acmHHu ya on: v« cox Va com v6 com .o cam vm cam vm 6:“ v6 ohm oHuom A. z uOuUOHUGH mouoom 3mm .3 "Ho u z a EH05 .m.m.¢ can Hem: :09..qu sawuoaouuoo .mH 0.309 108 instrument had a factor structure similar to the subscale scoring scheme, were not supported. A factor structure was found, however, which does support the contention that re- sponses to this instrument are situation specific. Hypotheses III and IV were related to the reliability of the scale. The hypothesis that the instrument was stable over a one-week test-retest period was supported. The in- strument was found to be internally consistant with a relia- bility of 0.74. The last six hypotheses were related to the validity of the scale. Hypothesis V, which indicated that the instru- ment could distinguish high empathizers from other persons was supported. The instrument was not able to distinguish between low empathizers and the norm group (Hypothesis VI). Hypothesis VII, indicating that the instrument could distinguish the effects of training known to be effective in increasing affective sensitivity, was supported. The hypoth- esis (VIII) that subjects scores are more similar to persons of the same educational level or profession than to persons of dissimilar education or profession also was supported. The last two hypothesis, that scores would be correla- ted with the earlier version (Form C) and with the "thinker" type on the MBTI were not supported. The meaning and significance of these results are dis- cussed in Chapter V. 109 CHAPTER V DISCUSSION AND CONCLUSION Summary This study began on the premise that the measurement of affective sensitivity, a necessary component of empathy, offered some distinct advantages as compared to the direct measurement of empathy. The literature on empathy presents three ways in which the presence of empathy is indicated. These ways include a cognitive response, a mixed cognitive-affective response or simply an affective response. The cognitive indicator is an awareness of the impact that one has on others or the ability to predict how another person is likely to feel or behave according to the situation. Affective-cognitive indicators require that a person have feelings along with another person as well as a cognitive grasp of the separat- ness from that person's experience. In this instance the affective response to the other persons situation mediates understanding. The definitions which include affective re- sponse as an indicator of empathy require a similarity of feeling between the subject and the other person or object for empathy to be present. The studies reported which focused on empathy in 110 interpersonal processes employed one or more of these indi- cations of empathy as the basis for the conclusions reached. These studies usually employed a research instrument, which operationally defined empathy based upon some assumption of what behaviors indicated its presence. A number of differ- ent instruments have been developed to measure empathy--a1- though little or no correspondence has been found between these instruments. The failure of these instruments to agree on what they measure is in part due to faulty instru- ment design and in part due to the different understandings of what exactly indicates the presence of empathy in a sit- uation. These failings make it difficult to understand ex- actly what is meant when a study reports on the relation- ship of some outcome variable and empathy. It is, therefore, difficult to decide what it is that a study may legitimately conclude. It is equally difficult to make generalizations based on more than one kind of study. In order to limit some of the ambiguity involved in the use and study of empathy, the use of the more limited construct of affective sensitivity has been proposed. This term, defined as the ability to detect and identify the feel- ing communication of another person, offers several advant- ages. These include reference to behaviors which are easy to describe, common usage of the term among most researchers, and the significant and necessary presence of these behav- iors in most definitions of empathy. While the term is not a synonym for empathy it does describe behaviors which make 111 up a major part of any definition of empathy, namely the awareness of feelings of others as well as the ability to identify what these feelings represent. An instrument for the measurement of affective sensi- tivity was developed by Kagan and his co-workers in 1962-1965. This instrument, the Affective Sensitivity Scale (A.S.S.) employed video tapes to provide a stimulus which approxi- mated the events of everyday existance, plus an objective multiple choice test. This instrument yielded a single scale score which has been demonstrated as both a reliable and valid measure of affective sensitivity (Kagan, et a1., 1967). While the instrument has proved useful for the purposes for which it was intended, namely the measurement of affec- tive sensitivity of counselors, several limitations are apparent. First, the audio and video quality of the instru— ment has deteriorated with time so that the present copies are at times nearly inaudible. Second, the persons included in the video portion are dated, both with respect to their mode of dress and to the nature of the concerns they present. In addition there is the need for an instrument useable in a wider range of settings other than just counseling, e.g. medical education, teacher training, as a diagnostic instru- ment in communication training, etc. In order to correct the shortcomings of this instru- ment (A.S.S. Form C) and to increase both its utility and discriminating powers a revised version of the Affective 112 Sensitivity Scale has been created by Kagan and Schneider. This new version, which has two comparable forms (Form D and Form B) has a greatly improved media fidelity, a wider range of stimulus situations, and a new scoring scheme which pro- vides a set of specific subscores in addition to the origin- al total score. The new scoring scheme scores item choices not simply as correct or incorrect but rather as preferred (correct), acceptable (somewhat correct) or other (not correct). The intention of this three level system of scoring was to in- dicate the extent to which the subject "risked" making the correct response. The subscale scoring represented all sets of items which had something in common, i.e. either they referred to persons who were either male or female, adult or child, client or interviewer or represented a particular setting, such as health care, education, informal, counsel- ing or psychotherapy. In addition to the thirteen subscales nine emperical scores were computed. These scales attempted to identify the response patterns of the subject, i.e. was there a preference for items which discounted the persons referred to, or did they indicate the subject was suspic- ious of the motives of the persons involved. The increased complexity of scoring necessitated computer scoring but also permitted both raw scale scores and standard scores to be provided to the user. The purpose of this study has been to investigate the structure, validity and reliability of this A.S.S. (Form D). 113 A study such as this was considered necessary in order to demonstrate the viability of the instrument as a research tool and as a measure of affective sensitivity. The preliminary stages of this research involved sev- eral revisions of the instrumentflsscoring key--in which the original scoring suggested by the instruments authors was revised to increase the discrimination ability of the scale. Four revisions were carried out, the first based on reference to a group of subjects acknowledged to be high empathizers and the last to optimize the effects resulting from training. Item analysis performed on the fourth revi- sion indicated that this form of the instrument and its scoring has a mean item difficulty of 53 and a mean index of discrimination of 33. Raw data was obtained from over 40 scale users and was employed to develop norms for the instrument. Norms based on a population of 3103 subjects were computed as well as norms for every identificable sub- group within the population, which includes males, females, undergraduates, medical students, occupational therapy trainees, family practice residents, psychiatric residents, graduate students, para-professionals, clergy, and correc- tions workers. This normative data is listed in Appendix E. Three basic questions were addressed in this study. First whether the construct of affective sensitivity repre- sented a simple construct or whether it, like empathy, was situation dependent. The second question was whether the A.S.S. was a reliable measure of affective sensitivity, 114 i.e. was it stable over time and was it a homogeneous measure of one behavior. The third question asked whether the in- strument was a valid measure of affective sensitivity, i.e. did it measure what it purported to measure. To answer these three general questions ten testable research hypoth- eses were stated and tested. Structure of the Instrument The first research question asked if the factor struc- ture of the instrument paralled the subscale scoring scheme. The implicit interest in this question was twofold: (1) could the subscale scoring procedure, based on specific settings or situations depicted in the stimulus part of the instrument, be supported as having some basis in fact, and therefore, (2) was the construct of affective sensitivity situation specific. Two forms of factor analysis were em- ployed to answer this question. The first method empiric- ally determined a set of factors to fit the instrument and the second method tested an ad hoc number of factors to see if they would adequately describe the underlying factor structure. This analysis indicated that the factor struc- ture of the instrument did not parallel the subscale scor- ing. Twenty-three factors were found, although only one accounted for an appreciable part of the variance and even then only 31% of it. The remaining 22 factors each account- ed for 6% or less of the variance. Hence the instrument has one general factor and many specific factors. These 115 factors all had one thing in common, however. They were loaded most heavily by pairs of items which were derived from individual scenes or episodes in the film part of the instrument. Hence responses to the instrument were gener- ally specific to each scene. The first research question was answered by affirming the situation specificity of the instrument although this specificity did not parallel the subscale scoring procedures. Reliability of the Instrument Two types of reliability studies were performed on the instrument, test-retest and internal homogeneity. The test- retest study which involved a retest period of one week for 20 subjects indicated that the instrument is stable over this short period of time; the correlation between the two administrations on the total score was 0.64. This appreci- able correlation and a net gain score on the total scale of 4.6 may indicate that some learning occurs or that memory of the items increases the likelihood of a correct response on retest. The second form of reliability testing employed was the computation of Chronbach's Alpha for both the total scale and all subscales. A total scale alpha of 0.74 in- dicates that the instrument is reasonably homogeneous; there- fore it measures essentially one behavior. However, the lowinter-item correlations (<0.1) indicates that each item varies considerably with respect to all other items, and 116 that each item may be measuring slightly different things. The Chronbach alpha scores for the other scales were pro- portionately smaller--the fewer the items which made up a subscale, the lower the alpha. Hence the subscales are less internally consistent than is the total scale. Validity of the Instrument Six hypotheses were tested in order to determine if the A.S.S. (Form D) did in fact measure what it purported to measure. Of particular interest was whether it measured the same behavior as the earlier (Form C) version, which had already been demonstrated as a valid measure of affective sensitivity. It was expected that the instrument would be able to differentiate both identified high and low empathizers from the norm group. A t-test for independent means was compu- ted which indicated significant differences between the means scores of the high empathizer referent group (N = 18) and the norm group. While these differences were statistically siginficant, their magnitude was generally less than one standard deviation (~0.7 SD), which is not as large as one might expect for a powerful instrument. No statistical differences were observed between the low empathizer refer- ent group (N = 8) and the norm group, although the 117 differences were all negative and about half a standard deviation in magnitude. A form of construct validity was demonstrated by com- paring the pre and post test scores of subjects involved in training previously demonstrated as likely to increase affective sensitivity. A significant positive difference was observed for the group as a whole as well as for most all the individual workshops. A positive gain score of 4.8 was noted overall and 5.3 for the programs which em- ployed IPR. Several of the populations reported no change in scores although none decreased. A subsequent test of two additional groups indicated no change, although this was consistent with previous experience with these particu- lar groups, which also indicated no change. Hence it was still concluded that the A.S.S. (Form D) was able to dis- tinguish the effects of training known to be effective in increasing affective sensitivity. The significance of these results may be questioned by the large gain score observed during the test-retest period of one week. This gain, about 4.6 pOints in magnitude, is about the same size ob- served for these groups. However, the majority of this training involved at least a month and its likely that mem- ory effects would diminish over this time period. For this assumption to be tested would require a test-retest analy- sis to be done with at least a one month lapse between testing. It was also hypothesized that the A.S.S. would produce 118 scores which were more consistant with an individual's peer group than any other dissimilar group. The population con- sidered in this study was already relatively homogeneous in that the vast majority of subjects were college educated (or in cOllege) and were mostly members of the helping pro- fession or in programs which were likely to lead to these professions. Seven distinct groups of subjects were ident- ified: college undergraduates, undergraduate social work students, first year medical students, family practice res- idents, psychiatry residents, occupational therapy trainees, and corrections workers. Comparison of the mean scores of these groups (based on 95% confidence intervals) indicated that the group means differed and for the most part they did not overlap. Hence it would appear that subjects do have scores which are more indicative of their own referent group than to other homogeneous groups. The relationship between the A.S.S. (Form D) and its predecessor (Form C) was determined by computing the corr- elation between scores obtained by subjects on both instru- ments. One hundred fifteen subjects were involved in this study which indicated that no significant positive correla- tions existed between these two instruments. The correla- tion which was found was negative although weak (rho for the total scale score was -0.15). This lack of positive correlation indicates that the two instruments do not mea- sure the same behaviors and does not provide support for this indicator of the validity of the A.S.S. (Form D) as 119 a measure of affective sensitivity. The final test for validity was based on the predicted correlation between those typed as "feelers” on the Myers- Briggs Type Indicator (MBTI) and their scores on the A.S.S. (Form D). Two types of correlations were considered-—the correlation between being typed either as a "thinker" or a "feeler" and scores on the A.S.S. and the correlation be- tween raw scores on all the MBTI scales and A.S.S. scores. A weak positive correlation was observed between the A.S.S. scores and those typed as feelers-~although the magnitudes were usually less than 0.2. The correlations between raw MBTI scores and A.S.S. scores were more significant. Essen- tially, zero correlations were observed between the four Thinking/Feeling and Sensing/Intuition raw scores while the Extroversion/Introversion and Judging/Perception indicated moderate correlations. In particular Extroversion and Judg- ing had positive correlations and the corresponding Intro- version and Perception were negatively correlated. This test of validity therefore does not support the hypothesis that those who are indicated as "feelers" on the MBTI are more likely to have higher scores on the A.S.S. than do the "thinker" types. Conclusions Based on the foregoing results the following conclu- sions may be drawn. 1. The subscale scoring of the A.S.S. (Form D) does 120 not reflect the internal structure of the test. The respon- ses of subjects as recorded by the subscale do not represent a consistent pattern of responding. However responses to the instrument appear to be situation specific in that re- sponses to items are frequently consistent across those items belonging to the same scene. 2. The instrument does appear to be reliable in that its test-retest reliability is 0.64 and its index of homog- eniety (Chronbach's alpha) is 0.74. For a test-retest period of one week there is however apparent learning, re- sulting in a significant gain score. This suggests that a longer test-retest period may be necessary to measure accur- ately the degree of learning. 3. The instrument is able to distinguish identified high empathizers from persons in general. On the other hand, it does not distinguish low empathizers from all-others, al- though the present results suggests that it may be able to do this for large samples. 4. The A.S.S. (Form D) is able to distinguish the effects of training in programs known to be effective inin- creasing affective sensitivity. 5. Members of homogeneous groups will have mean scores on the A.S.S. (Form D) which are distinguishable from groups of persons who differ either by education or profession. 6. On the basis of the analysis made in this study the A.S.S. (Form D) does not appear to measure the same be- haviors as does its predecessor, the A.S.S. (Form C). Scores 121 on Form D are not correlated with the earlier form, or at best, are negatively correlated with.it. 7. The Affective Sensitivity Scale (Form D) does not distinguish between those typed as "thinking" (T) or "feel- ing" (F) on the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator. There is how- ever, a positive correlation between the raw scores on the Extroversion (E) and Judging (J) typologies. Taken together, the last five conclusions may be em- ployed to discribe the validity of the Affective Sensitivity Scale (Form D). Since the instrument is not correlated with its predecessor it is therefore not a measure of af- fective sensitivity. Yet it is capable of distinguishing between high and low empathizers and it does detect differ- ences due to training known to be effective in increasing affective sensitivity. Hence it must be a measure of be- havior which are related to empathy. What these behaviors are may not be directly inferred from the above conclusions. Since it has been recognized that empathy is a global con- struct many behaviors are likely entailed in this term. Affective sensitivity represents the ability to detect and identify the feelings of other persons, and is likely one of these behaviors. Some of the other behaviors which might also be included in empathy are likely to be cognitive in nature, such as the ability to recognize the meanings feelings have for different people, or the likely feelings associated with particular experiences or events . In any case 122 this instrument would not appear to be a valid measure of affective sensitivity although there is some support for it as a valid measure of some other component of empathy. What this component is has not been determined by this study. Discussion It would seem clear on the basis of these findings that this study has'not found support for the A.S.S. (Form D) as a valid measure of affective sensitivity. On the other hand there have been identified positive features of the instrument. It would appear that it is a reliable mea- sure, stable over time and internally consistent, able to distinguish between persons identified as high and low empathizers and sensitive to the effects of training in interpersonal communication skills. The instrument has also been reported by users as confirming their perceptions of trainees' abilities, hence as having some kind of face validity. While the instrument has not been demonstrated as a valid measure of affective sensitivity, it does mea- sure a behavior or behaviors which appear to be correlated with empathy. Some clues as to what it measures may be in- ferred by considering how it is similar to and in what ways it differs from the earlier Form C version. 123 Comparison Between Form C and Form D Form C of the Affective Sensitivity Scale (or more correctly its predecessor Form B, of which it is a somewhat abbreviated version) was developed with the intention of having one correct and two incorrect choices to each item. The correct responses were based on recall data provided by the persons involved (Campbell, 1968) or on the judgment of experts in the areas of counseling and psychotherapy. The incorrect responses were generated by the idea of being wrong--of reflecting feelings which were not present. Form D items were also derived from recall data but the alternatives were created with the intent to distinguish the response preferences of the subject (Schneider, 1977) and not simply to determine if the subject could identify the correct from the incorrect item choices. Hence some- times all of the item choices were somewhat correct. The choice was not so much between correct or incorrect but be- tween items which differed in the extent to which they re- ferred to the central feelings or concerns. The other item choices may have all been correct but they referred to feelings or concerns which were not central to the situation. In the final scoring revision these "less central" item choices were usually scored as "other" (incorrect). A second way in which Form D and Form C differ is the construction of the item choices themselves. According to Schneider (1977) some of these differences are as follows: 1. Items on Form C are feeling statements. Those on 124 Form D are statements about feelings. For example consider items from each of these instruments: Form C: "I feel rejected and empty inside. Am I unloveable?" Form D: "He can't really understand what it's like to lose a year and a half of your life that way." 2. Form C emphasizes the importance of acknowledging negative feelings about the self, while Form D attaches greater importance to positive feelings. A comparison of items which refer to either positive feelings (relief, liking, happy, pleased, etc.) or negative feelings (embar- assed, scared, hurt, rejected, etc.) indicates a large dis- crepancy between the two versions of the instrument. In Form C most of the item choices which refer to negative feelings are the correct choices while this is true only half the time on Form D. However for items which refer to positive feelings they are likely to be scored as correct on Form D and incorrect on Form C. A recent study employ- ing the A.S.S. (Form D) by Ingram (1977) indicated that "a chi square analysis found positive affect to be over repre- sented in the multiple choice alternatives that were weight- ed as most accurate ("preferred") and hostile affect to be overrepresented in the error alternatives." Hence FOrm D is biased toward the acknowledgement of positive affect while Form C is biased toward negative affect. 3. While Form C was constructed with only three item alternatives, Form D has five. The items on C are more consistent and are statements of feelings. Form D items 125 are more varied and while some statements of feelings are included more often they are about feelings or about cog- nitions. 4. Form C items refer only to the client. In the case of Form D a client is not present in all the episodes. In addition items typically refer to the interviewer (therapist, counselor, teacher, etc.) as well as to the client. Hence all persons represented by the episodes may be the object of an item and the subject is encouraged to focus on all persons rather than just one. 5. The instruments are not similar in the difficulty of selecting an appropriate alternative. Form C focuses only on the client and asks the subject to choose an alter- native from three possible of which only one is correct. Form D asks the subject to focus on all parties in the scene and to select from five possible alternatives the one which is most correct. Because of the greater number of items and.the generally more difficult nature of the items them- selves the instrument may well be sensitive to the subject's ability to read and to make quick decisions. This is espec- ially significant since both Form C and Form D provide about the same time (30 seconds per item) in which the subject is to make a choice. In summary Form D would appear to be a more difficult instrument to take (this is substantiated by frequent com- ments made by subjects about the frustration they experienced while taking the test), which requires the subject to be more 126 attentive, to make decisions based on five alternatives rather than three and between alternatives which all may appear as correct. The test is likely to favor those who read fast, as well as with good cOmprehension (Foreign‘ students and those for whom English is a second language report difficulty reading all the alternatives in the time limits). Most important, the items in Form D are about feelings or thoughts rather than statements of feelings. The lack of reference to what the person is feeling would work against the instrument as a measure of affective sen- sitivity. Finally the instrument is biased in the Opposite direction from Form C in that it is more likely to refer to positive affect while C refers to negative affect. For ex— ample, in Form C 94% (31 of 33) of the item choices which refer to negative affect (anger, hate, sadness, etc.) are scored_as correct, while with Form D only 58% (31 of 53) of such items are scored as correct. On the other extreme only 20% (3 of 15) of the item choices which refer to positive affect (love, warmth, joy, etc.) on Form C are scored as cor- rect, while 80% (20 of 25) of such item choices on Form D are scored as correct. Other evidence which supports the A.S. S. (Form D) is a measure of a cognitive function is the correlation between the scale and the Extorversion (E) and Judging (J) raw scores on the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator. The Extorversion typology is related to a person's ten- dency to look outward as a means of gathering data. 127 They are turned toward other people. The Judging function is often related to a person's willingness to make decisions based on available data. Hence the positive correlation that exits with regard to these two MBTI types suggests that the instrument measures behaviors which are in the cognitive decision making domain. This would logically follow from the requirement that subjects distinguish a correct alter- native from other alternatives which are not quite incorrect, but rather less relevant or less central to the concerns contained within the stimulus scenes. Present Utility of the Affective Sensitivity Scale (Form D) When the A.S.S. (Form D) was created it was the au- thors' expectation that it would be employed for formative evaluation to encourage self-knowledge and self-exploration through feedback. The instrument has been successful in meeting this goal as is indicated by the large number of users and the large number of administrations. Reports from users support the usefulness of the instrument for formative evaluation and the present levels of validity and reliability are acceptable for this purpose. Further re- search on the instrument is necessary in order to understand better what the precise qualities it measures. The instru- ment in its present form would not appear to be appropriate as a summative evaluative device, since it lacks the con- struct validity necessary for decision making or criterion setting evaluation. 128 Continued use of the instrument in its present form as a measure of affective sensitivity however, is probably not appropriate. This observation is based on the findings of this study that the A.S.S. (Form D) has not been demon- strated as a valid measure of this behavior. It does appear to measure a behavior which is likely a cognitive component of empathy, the nature of which remains to be delineated. As a formative evaluative device the instrument would ap- pear to be useful and its use as a stimulant to self-dis- covery is supported. The subscale scoring scheme would appear to offer some benefits as a heuristic device, but subjects should be in- formed of the lack of validity of these scales. The empir- ical scales would appear to be slightly more valid al- though this too has not been established. The results of this study indicate that further research on the instrument is desirable,especially research which would identify what the instrument measures. In addition, if the instrument (at least the film portion) is to be used as a measure of affective sensitivity then modification of the multiple choice test portion is seen here as necessary. Implications for Further Research on the Present Form of the Instrument Further research on the present version of the A.S.S. (Form D) would be fruitful, particularly if it would seek to determine what it measures and how this relates to affective sensitivity or empathy. Some possible avenues 129 for this kind of research are as follows: It has been reported that the complexity of the item choices favors subjects with high reading comprehension and speed. The impact of these variables on scale scores could be assessed by correlating scores on such instruments as the Scholastic AptitudeTest (SAT). (Verbal scores) which are available for many High School graduates, or with other measures of reading skill, such as those routinely employed by universities as part of the admissions proced- ures. At Michigan State University all incoming freshmen are required to take a reading comprehension test and these scores could be available for correlation with A.S.S. scores. Another way to identify the cognitive aspects of the A.S.S. (Form D) would be to obtain I.Q. scores, particularly from an instrument such as the Weschler Adult Intelligence Scale. The Weschler offers particular advantages because of its subscale scoring which includes six scales of Verbal behavior and six of Performance behavior (Zimmerman and Woo- Sam, 1973). Correlation between these subscales and the A.S.S. would provide insight into the manner in which sub- jects process information and their scores on the A.S.S. It would be expected that verbal scores would correlate highly with the A.S.S. (Form D). Correlation of other personality measures with the A.S.S. would indicate what personality characteristics are likely to be reflected in scale scores. Some studies of this nature have already begun. James Ayres (University 130 of Minnesota) has correlated the A.S.S. (Form D) with Tell- egen's Differential Personality Questionnaire (DPQ) which suggests possible correlations with Social Closeness. Other instruments which might also be considered are the Allport-Vernon-Lindzey Study of Values, Edwards Personal Preference Schedule and the Accurate Empathy Scale (AU). Since the empirical subscales and indices appeared to measure different behaviors from the total scale (see Appendix G for inter-scale correlations) these scales might be expanded and/or further analyzed. The empirical scales were created by noting trends which appeared to run through the item choices, so for example, the Discounting subscale was generated by collecting all items which implied that the Subject believed other persons to be unable to take care of themselves or to know their own feelings. The present scales could be refined and analyzed further. Item correlations and factor analysis performed on this instrument have only considered the items as units-- yet the item choices may be a more fundamental unit of the test. Hence factor analysis based on item choices might reveal a factor structure underlying the items themselves which is independent of the scoring key. Correlation be- tween item choices and the earlier Form C version would pro- vide further insight into the ways that these instruments are either similar or different. In general, given the lack of homogeniety of the item choices themselves, correlations between any other instrument and the A.S.S. (Form D) should 131 be made both with scale scores and specific item choices. It may indicate that the instrument is capable of discimin- ations which the present scoring key does not permit. Implications for Modification of the A.S.S. (Form D) Based on the conclusions in this study there are two approaches indicated to modify the new version (Form D) of the Affective Sensitivity Scale, which promise increases in this instrument's power. One approach is to focus on increasing its effectiveness as a cognitive measure of em— pathy--that which it does to some extent at present. A second approach would be to make the changes necessary to create a new instrument for the measurement of affective sensitivity--the original purpose for which this instru- ment was developed. Continued development of the present form of the in- strument would serve several purposes. First of all, the instrument does make distinctions among subjects which ap- pear to offer some heuristic value. Second, the instrument has been extensively investigated already and there may be some advantages in captializing on the research already completed. Third, the instrument likely measures behaviors which are of interest in the development of communication skills. The first change which would increase the power of the instrument as a cognitive measure would be to eliminate all item choices which are statements of feelings. This 132 would remove completely the affective component. Another change which appears to be desirable would be to reduce some of the ambiguity in item choices by elimin- ating one of the five multiple choice alternatives. The item to be removed should be the second most correct alter- native, so that the distinction between the correct and incorrect alternatives becomes more clear. The factor analytic studies indicated a single primary factor which accounts for only 31% of the variances of this instrument. Item choices could be modified or eliminated to increase the strength of this factor. This would likely increase the homogeniety of the instrument as well as remove the extensive sub-factor structure which underlies it. It is expected that this factor is a cognitive factor and hence such a change would increase the power of the instrument as a cognitive measure. Other modifications which would increase the utility of the present A.S.S. (Form D) as a measure of the cognitive component of empathy would be to define more clearly the cognitive aspects of empathy and focused on them to write items which reflect these aspects. For example, the items could be statements which a subject might make in response to the stimulus situation which would reflect either an in- tervention Strategy or a label which indicate the nature of the problem. This would provide the subject with feedback on the modes in which he or she perceives interpersonal sit- uations and permit comparison with other persons recognized 133 as experts, or with persons in the same or different pro- fessions. The use of the A.S.S. (Form D) as a measure of the cognitive aspects of empathy would appear to offer some ben- efits in communication skill training, or in professional de- velopment. Continued use of this instrument as a measure of a cognitive component of empathy might suggest that a new name which reflects its cognitive nature might be more appro- priate, such as the Cognitive Measure of Interpersonal Sen- sitivity (CMIS) or other such name. A second direction the modifications might take would be to insure that the A.S.S. (Form D) were a measure of af- fective sensitivity. The stimulus material (film portion of the scale) would appear adequate as a stimulus in that it does provide a wide range of situations which are emotion- ally intense and which lend themselves to questions about affect. Modification would require developing a multiple choice test along the lines of the earlier Form C version (this task has already been started by the scales authors, Kagan and Schneider). Changes would involve developing item choices which are statements of feelings, and with one clear- 1y correct and two incorrect alternatives. As with Form C the items could be of two types, items which refer to the person's awareness of themselves (selfawareness) and items which reflect the person's feelings about the other person(s) in the scene (relationship). This kind of modification would lead to the advantages of the earlier scale plus the vi. ‘s. 134 increased applicability of the instrument because of its wider range of stimulus situations. The instrument might also be modified to permit it to determine response style. If the multiple choice portion of the instrument were developed more along the lines of Por- ter's Helping Relationship Inventory (Pfeiffer and Jones, 1973) then this would be a possibility. Porter's instrument provided five different response modes to each (written) episode, to which the subject provided a rank ordering of the responses. Because the episodes in the A.S.S. are about dialogues between other persons and not statements directed toward the subject the responses would have to be of a dif- ferent nature. The subject might be asked what statement he or she would prefer in order to provide assistance, clar— ification, or direction to the subjects in the episodes. The subject could rank order several different intervention strategies resulting in a preference score for one type strategy over others. Another approach within this same format would be to provide alternatives much like the pre- sent kind, although structured such that they represent dis- tinct attitudes of the perceiver, such as suspiciousness, discounting, affectively, cognitively, feeling awareness, sensitizing, repressing of feelings, etc. These various responses would need to be plausible interpretations of the situations presented and the subject would obtain a profile which indicated their preferred mode of responding to inter- personal situations. 135 Whether further research procedes along the lines of developing a cognitive measure of empathy or a: measure of affective sensitivity, or a combination of these two, the stimulus portion of the A.S.S. (Form D) provides an adequate base for stimulating research. What has been learned in the course of this study and what was already known from earlier studies on the instrument increases the likelihood that more reliable and valid instruments will be created. The need for such instrumentation has been demonstrated both by the considerable use of the present instrument in its develop- mental stages and in the number of studies both about and with the instrument which have already begun. The Affective Sensitivity Scale (Form D) in its present form does have heuristic value in providing subjects with a stimulus for personal and professional growth. Further research is desirable to identify the specific behaviors that the instrument measures and to determine the degree to which these behaviors are a necessary part of empathy. APPENDIX APPENDIX A AFFECTIVE SENSITIVITY SCALE FORM C Reproduced here with the permission of the authors. AFFECTIVE SENSITIVITY SCALE Instructions YOu will be viewing short scenes of actual counseling sessions. You are to identify what feelings the clients have toward themselves and toward the counselors they are working with. Although in any one scene a client may exhibit a variety of feelings, for the purpose of this instrument you are to concentrate on identifying his last feelings in the scene. 0n the following pages are multiple choice items consisting of three responses each. Most scenes have two items, but a few have one or three items. After you View each scene, you are to read the items and ask yourself the following question: If the client were to view this same scene, and if he were completely open and honest with him- self, (i.e., if he could identify his real feelings) which of these three responses would he use to describe his feelings? After you decide which response accurately describes what the client is actually feeling either about himself or the counselor he is with, indicate your choice on the answer sheet. Here is a sample item: CLIENT I Scene 1 $232.1 1. This exploring of my feelings is good. It makes me feel good. 2. I feel very sad and unhappy. 3. I'm groping and confused; I can't bring it (all together. After you had viewed Scene 1 for CLIENT I, you would read these three state- ments (Item 1) and would then decide which one best states what the client would say about his own feelings after viewing the same scene. For example, if you decide number two best states what the client is feeling, you would then find the number 1 on your answer sheet and darken in the space for number two . 1. 1 :::: 2 [III 3 :::: 4 :::: 5 :::: we will only make use of the first three answer spaces following each item on your answer sheet. Remember you are to concentrate on the latter part of each scene in deter- mining the most accurate description of the client's feelings. After you view the appropriate scenes, you will have thirty seconds to 136 137 answer each of the first twelve items. For each of the remaining items, you will be allowed twenty seconds. CAUTION: The item numbers on your answer sheet go across the page, not down the page as you would usually expect! AFFECTIVE SENSITIVITY SCALE REVISED FORM B CLIENT I Scene 1 Item 1 l. I feel sorry for my husband and the relationship we have. 2. I don't really understand what I feel. Yet, I do feel guilty about creating pain in others which returns to me. 3. I feel pleased at seeing a possible relationship between my feelings of anger and pain. Item 2 1. He (counselor) doesn't have to like me. I just want him to agree with me and tell me I'm right. 2. I'm trying to please you. Do you like me? 3. He's really understanding me now. CLIENT I Scene 2 Item 3 l. I feel calm and collected. I just want to think for a while. 2. Yes, that is when I get angry. I see it all clearly now. 3. I feel anxious and stimulated. Item 4 1. I'll pretend I'm agreeing with him (counselor), but I don't see the connection at all. 2. I like what he's doing. I don't feel as uncomfortable now. 3. I wish he would stop pushing me in this direction. .138 CLIENT II Scene 1 ' Item 5 1. I'm pleased, happy; I feel good all over! 2. It was brought right back, that amazes me, but it hits quite bad too. It hurts! 3. I'm not bothered by this. I can handle it. I'm confident. Item 6 1. He's (counselor) caught me; careful, I'm not sure I want that. 2. I like him- He's trying to make the situation a little lighter and made me feel better about it. 3. I don't feel he understands. He's sarcastic. I don't like that. CLIENT II Scene 2 Item 7 1. I feel a little uneasy and self-conscious, but not much. 2. This scares me. I feel frightened! 3. I feel flirtatious. I like this! Item 8 l. I feel a little bit embarrassed, but that's all right as long as I can keep my composure. 2. I have a feeling of sadness. 3. I feel flustered and embarrassed. Item 9 1. He's asking for some touchy material, but that's all right. It's about time he knew. 2. He's being very frank and open! I'm not sure I want that. _ 3. I'want him.to leave me alone--I want out of here. I don't like this. 139 CLIENT II Scene 3 Item 10 1. I'm getting so much attention. I really enjoy this. It makes me feel good. 2. I'm scared by what I'm feeling. I feel embarrassed and threatened. - 3. I have the feeling that what I wanted was wrong, and I'm.a little ashamed of myself. Item 11 I. This is good. We're really moving into my feelings. 2. He's too perceptive; he's looking right through me. 3. He's getting a little sticky; I'm not sure I like that. CLIENT III Scene 1 Item 12 l. I feel protective and defensive of what people may think about my family. 2. All this seems so pointless! I'm puzzled and bored. 3. We're having a nice conversation. Some of these things really make me think. Item 13 1. This guy (counselor) embarrasses me with the questions he asks. 2. The questions he asks really make me think. I'm not sure I like that. 3. I can't follow this guy's line of thought. What's he trying to do? CLIENT IV Scene 1 Item 14 1. I'm concerned about my physical condition. I'm worried about it. 2. I want pity. I want her to think "oh, you poor boy". 3. I feel good--nothing's bothering me, but I enjoy talking. Item 15 1. She's too young to be counseling, and she's a girl. I'm not sure I like this. 2. She likes me; I know she does. 3. I'd like her to think I'm great. 140 CLIENT IV Scene 2 1.93.1.6. 1. I'm a little annoyed with my family's ambitions for me. 2. That's a hell of a lot to ask! It makes me mad! 3. I feel sorry for myself, and I want others to feel the same. Item 17 1. She (counselor) really understands me! She's with me now. 2. I don't feel much either way towards the counselor; she's not important to me. 3. I wonder if she appreciates the pressure that's put on me? CLIENT IV Scene 3 Item 18 1. This whole thing just makes me feel sad and unhappy. 2. It kind of angers me that they don't appreciate me when I feel I did my best. I wish I could tell them off. 3. No matter how well I do, I'm always criticized. It doesn't bother me too much though because I know that I did my best. Item 19 1. I can tell she understands what I'm saying. She's really with me. 2. I wish I could get out of here; I don't like her. 3. Understand what I'm saying; I want her to know how I feel. 141. CLIENT IV Scene 4 Item 20 1. I really want to be successful, and somehow I know that I can be. 2. That makes me feel kind of sad, unhappy. I don't want to believe that it's true--I want to be good. 3. I don't know what I feel here. It's all very confusing. Item 21 l. I feel neutral towards her here. I'm not paying any attention to her. 2. Please feel sorry for me and try to help me. I wish she would praise me. 3. I like talking to her. She can be trusted even to the point of telling her how I really feel about myself. CLIENT V Scene 1 Item 22 l. I feel rejected and empty inside. Am I unloveable? 2. I feel a little lonely. I want my boy friend to pay a little more attention to me. 3. I really don't feel much here; I'm just kind of talking to fill up space. 15333—3 1. Please say it isn't fair, Mr. Counselor. 2. He really understands me. I can tell him anything. 3. I'm not sure I care what he says. It's kind of unimportant to me what he feels about me at this time. 142 CLIENT V Scene 2 Item 24 1. I'm afraid of marriage--insecure; it might not work out, and I'd be lost. 2. I really can give him all the affection he needs, I feel I'm.a worthwhile person to be desired. He wouldn't dare step out on me. 3. I'm really not too worried; it'd all work out in the end even if we have to go to a marriage counselor. Item 25 l. I don't care if he (counselor) can help me or not. I'm not sure I want his help. 2. He's so sympathetic. That makes me feel good. 3. Can you help me? CLIENT V Scene 3 Item 26 l. I feel I have some need to be liked, but it's not real strong. 2. I'm not loveable; I don't really like myself. 3. I'm a good person; I'm loveable. Down deep I know I am. Item 27 l. I feel dejected, kind of insecure. I want to be likeable! 2. My main concern is that it's hard for me to take criticism. I usually think of myself as perfect. 3. I feel a little sad about all this; I do kind of . want people to like me. Item 28 1. He thinks well of me; I know he does, I can tell. 2. I want the counselor to really like me, but I'm not sure he does. 3. I like it when he asks questions like that. They make me really think about deeper things. 1143 CLIENT‘V Scene 4 $592.12. 1. I wouldn't want to be treated like he treats Mother, but I don't mind him (stepfather) too much. 2. I feel very little emotion about anything at this point. 3. I hate hhm (stepfather)! Item 30 1. Boy, I'm happy that he (counselor) agrees with me. He sympathizes with me. I feel completely accepted. 2. I'm embarrassed to tell the counselor how strong my feelings really are. 3. I'm not sure he'll be able to help me much after all. I'll just have to work this out by myself. CLIENT V Scene 5 Item 31 l. I'm.kind of feeling sorry for myself, but I'm not really too worried. 2. I want to move out of the house as soon as possible. I feel I would be better off on my own. 3. My own parents don't want me; I feel cut off and hurt. Item 32 l. I don't feel he's (counselor) helpful at all, and if he can't help me and see my side, I'm not going to like him either. 2. He's got me in a spot, but I feel I can still get him.to see me as a good girl who is persecuted. 3. I wish the counselor were my father. He's listening; he understands how I feel. CLIENT VI Scene 1 Item 33 l. Disapprove! She'd kill me! 2. I feel jovial; this is real interesting. 3. I'm not sure how she would feel but the whole idea of her finding out excites me. Item 34 1. He (counselor) understands me completely. He certainly is relaxed and comfortable. 2. I really don't care what he feels about me. I just want someone to talk to--anyone will do. 3. I was wondering how he would feel about me and what I'm.aaying. 144 CLIENT VI Scene 2 £93.32 1. I think my brother is 0.x. We have fun together. 2. I don't know what I'm saying here. I'm a little mixed up and confused. 3. I'm saying something that's important to me. I like Doug. CLIENT VI Scene 3 Item 36 1. This is very confusing for me. I'm not sure I understand what is going on. 2. This is how I really feel, I'm kind of starting to be myself. 3. I'm just talking to be talking here; this really doesn't mean much to me. Item 37 1. I guess he's (counselor) all right, but I'm still not sure he understands me. 2. Let's get going. I'm.impatient! I want to move to more important matters. 3. I feel comfortable with him. He understands me. CLIENT VI Scene 4 Item 38 1. I love my brother, but not romantically. We just have a good brother-sister relationship. 2.- I don't know about feeling this way about Doug; it feels so good, but it concerns me too. 3. I feel better about my relationship with Doug now. It helps to get it out in the open. Now I feel it's all right. 145 CLIENT VI Scene 5 Item 39 1. I'm not feeling much of anything here. I'm just kind of talking to be talking. 2. I'm.mad at everyone at this point and don't know which way to turn; I guess I'm mad at myself too. 3. Now I'm talking about things that are real. I'm not on stage anymore. She is a louse! Item 40 1. He (counselor) feels she's a bad person too. I can tell; he agrees with me. 2. Don't you agree with me? I want to know what you think. 3. He thinks this all sounds petty. He doesn't understand. CLIENT VII Scene 1 Item 41 l. I felt angry with my mother, but this made me feel guilty. I needed to make an excuse for her. 2. I'm really not angry with mother. It's not her fault. 3. I'm in a very passive mood. I'm just relaxing and talking about things that interest me. Item 42 1. This counselor is all right. I feel I can confide in him. ,2. I feel uncomfortable. I'm not sure what this counselor wants me to do. 3. I feel he wants me to talk about myself, but I don't care. I'm going to talk about what I want to talk about. 146 CLIENT VII ' Scene 2 Item 43 1. I'm very sensitive; I'm very easily hurt. 2. I'm somewhat sensitive and easily hurt, but not deeply so. 3. I'm not sensitive or easily hurt at all. I just like to make people think I am. Item 44 1. That makes me mad, I can do it--I know I can, but things just keep getting in my way. 2. It's really all his fault, if he just wouldn't have been such a joker. 3. This makes me feel guilty; I need to blame someone else instead of blaming myself. Item 45 1. I'm neutral towards the counselor. I don't care what he feels about me. 2. I'm afraid he doesn't like me and what Im saying about myself. I don't want him to be harsh with me. 3. He's easy to talk to. He understands what I'm like, and he still likes me. I can confide in him. CLIENT VIII Scene 1 Item 46 1. Say, this is all right. I like this. 2. I'm not feeling anything deeply.’ I know what I need! 3. ‘It's embarrassing and difficult. I feel a little annoyed. Item 47 l. I feel I can rely on this guy, so I'll let him talk and I'll just answer his questions. 2. I wonder what you think about this--please respond. Give me some help! 3. The counselor is a good guy. I like his questions; they make it easier for me. 147 CLIENT VIII Scene 2 Item 48 l. I feel very unhappy about what I may eventually have to do. 2. I don't know what I feel; I'm.confused about what I feel. ' 3. I'm damned uncomfortable; it's so confusing. I feel kind of 'blah' about it all. Item 49 1. He's (counselor) missing the point. He bugs me. 2. I can't really tell about this guy. I don't know how I feel about him. 3. He seems like a good guy. He asks nice questions. I like him. CLIENT IX Scene 1 151L533. 1. I'm not sure how I feel about this counselor. I don't feel one way or the other about him. 2. I like the counselor very much--he makes me feel good. 3. He understands me pretty well and is trying to help. I guess I kind of like him. CLIENT IX Scene 2 £92.51 1. Goody, goody people don't really know any better, so I can't be too disgusted with them, but it does make me angry. 2. I don't really mind people feeling superior to me. It just makes me a little angry. 3. It tears me up inside when people think they're better than I am. I want people to be the same as me. Item 52 1. I'm every bit as good as they are. I really feel I am. I know I am. 2. I kind of wished they liked me, but I can live without being a member of their group. 3. Those smart kids make me feel stupid. Item 53 l. I feel sorry for them; they just don't realize what -they're doing to people like me. 2. I feel I'm not as good as they are, and it really hurts when people act that way. 3. It makes me a little angry. I'm every bit as good as they are. 148 CLIENT IX Scene 3 Item 54 l. I feel a little insignificant, and this makes me a little unhappy. 2. I'm a nobody. I'm always left out. 3. I'm unhappy with school. That's what is really bothering me. Item 55 1. He (counselor) doesn't quite understand, but I don't care. It doesn't matter. 2. I don't feel one way or the other towards this counselor, we're just having a nice talk. 3. He (counselor) is really listening to me, and I feel he understands what I'm feeling. CLIENT X Scene 1 Item 56 1. I'm feeling scared, concerned. Is this for me? 2. I just feel uncertain about what to talk about. If I once get started, I'll be all right. 3. I feel very deeply depressed. Item 57 1. He (counselor) seems to be listening-~can he understand how I feel? 2. He's really with me. I can tell he understands me. 3. He doesn't keep things moving enough. I don't like that. CLIENT X Scene 2 - Item 58 1. I'd like to think I could make it, but I'm not sure. I feel inadequate. 2. I just have an I-don't-care feeling; that's my real attitude towards all of this. 3. I'm confused here. I really don't have any definite feelings. Item 59 l. I want to impress the counselor. I want him to believe I can do it. 2. He believes me; he thinks I can do it; I can tell. 3. I really don't care what the counselor thinks. It's not important to me. 149 CLIENT X Scene 3 Its—£29 1. What's the use of looking ahead? I'm scared to think about it. ' 2. I can accept my situation. Really, things aren't so bad. Things may bother mela little, but really not much. 3. I enjoy just living for today. Item 61 1. He's (counselor) all right. He really understands ' me. 2. Nobody can really understand this. I don't think he will be any different. 3. I don't care what he thinks or feels; he's not important to me anyway. CLIENT X Scene 4 Item 62 l. I feel somewhat unhappy. I don't like to feel this‘way. 2. There's something about me; I just don't fit in, and that makes me feel real inadequate. 3. In some instances, I'm unsure of myself. I'm afraid I'll do the wrong thing, but I can handle this just by avoiding these situations. CLIENT XI Scene 1 Item 63 1. I'm unhappy about all this, but I'm afraid to make a change. 2. It's not that I don't like school, it's just that I want to do the things I like most. 3. I'm.not the student type. School bores me, but it embarrasses me when I say it. Item 64 l. The counselor is a nice guy. I like him, and I think he likes me. 2. I wonder what the counselor thinks of me. He'll probably think less of me for saying this. 3. I don't care what he thinks of me. It doesn't really matter to me. 150 CLIENT XI Scene 2 Item 65 1. I've found some new dimensions. I like to feel that I can have some excitement, but this kind of scares me too. 2. This doesn't really mean much. I'm not feeling much of anything. 3. This makes me feel very guilty; I'm very ashamed. Item 66 l. I suppose he'll (counselor) tell me that's wrong, too. I'm not sure he understands me very well. 2. He's 0.K.; he's listening to what I have to say. He really understands me and my feelings. 3. I don't care what he thinks or feels; it's not important. I don't have any feelings towards the counselor. APPENDIX B AFFECTIVE SENSITIVITY SCALE EXAMINER'S MANUAL FORMS D AND E Reproduced here with the permission of the authors. AFFECTIVE SENSITIVITY SCALE EXAMINER'S MANUAL FORMS D & E © Copyright 1975 Normal I. Kagan, Ph.D. - John M. Schneider, Ph.D. © Copyright 1977 Normal I. Kagan, Ph.D. John M. Schneider, Ph.D. Not to be reproduced without explicit written permission of the authors. 151 152 Affective Sensitivity Scale Forms D & E Examiner's Manual Table of Contents A. INTRODUCTION Purpose Nature of examination procedures Materials Included Additional Materials Needed RECOMMENDED EXAMINATION PROCEDURES INSTRUCTION - FORM D 8 E INSTRUCTIONS FOR FEEDBACK: DEFINITION & EXPLANATION OF CATEGORIES EXAMINER'S COPY OF FORMS D &/OR E THE DEVELOPMENT OF THE AFFECTIVE SENSITIVITY SCALE: A DEVELOPMENTAL VIEW CURRENT NORMS: FORM D CURRENT NORMS: FORM E 153 AFFECTIVE SENSITIVITY SCALE EXAMINER'S MANUAL FORMS D & E by Norman 1. Kagan, Ph.D. John M. Schneider, Ph.D. INTRODUCTION Purpose: An evaluation instrument intended to measure one aspect of the examinee's sensitivity in interpersonal interactions. Nature of the Examination Procedure: Each scale consists of 30 film segments taken from various inter- personal encounters, ranging in time from eight seconds to approx- imately two minutes. Following each scene, the examinee answers multiple choice items designed specifically for that scene. Necessareraterials: l. Examiner's Manual 2. 16mm. color sound A.S.S. - Form D or E 3. Test booklets - Form D or E (one per examinee) 4. Answer sheet — IBM 5. 16mm. sound projector (if possible, the projector should be located so that the noise of its operation is minimized) 6. StOpwatch 7. Examining room minimizing visual and auditory distractions 8. Examination pencils B. 154 RECOMMENDED EXAMINATION PROCEDURES 1. Make the examinees as comfortable as possible prior to the start of testing. You may wish to share the following infor- mation to help them anticipate the experience in addition to instructing them to read the instructions at the beginning of the test booklet: a. They will be seeing a series of short segments from various interpersonal encounters. Some will be of teachers with students, doctors with patients, couples, families, class- rooms and psychotherapy. b. They will be asked: If the persons in the scene were being honest with themselves, "what do you think the persons are really saying to themselves at that point?" c. There are two and sometimes three items per scene. Please check carefully so that all items are answered. They are to give only one answer per item. d. Have the examinees note the "Confidentiality" statement in the booklet, which also appears at the beginning of the film. e. Despite the fact that this is a test, some of the scenes have a strong impact on the examinee. You may wish to offer them the opportunity to discuss their reactions with you afterwards. f. Be sure to use the appropriate answer sheets. Remind the examinees to place name and/or other appropriate identify- ing data on the answer sheets. DO NOT MARK THE QUESTION BOOKLET IN ANY WAY. g. If any examinee has a marked booklet give them the oppor- tunity to obtain a clean booklet, since such markings tend to be distracting (and of uncertain accuracy). Be sure the projector operator is alert to the end of a scene. The projector must be stopped within 2 to 4 seconds of the end of a scene. Previewing the film before showing for the first time can help the operator anticipate the end of a scene. Lights need to be off or dimmed during the playing of the film. Adequate light is necessary for answering the questions. It is possible to obtain one lignt level adequate both for viewing the film and answering the questions. Allow 39 seconds for each item. For all scenes, the total time between scenes will be 60 or 90 seconds, depending on the number of items. The instructor will need a copy of the exam to keep track. Use a stop watch or second hand on a watch or clock to 5. 155 keep track of the time. At the end of the time period, immed- iately start the projector. It is possible with the help of your audio-visual department to splice an amount of black film between scenes so timed (36 feet/minute) that the operator does not need to stop and start the projector ( - sorry, we will not permit videotape copies to be made from the film). The total time of test should be approximately Z§_minutes. As a result of the experiences of several users of the scale, we make the following additional suggestions: 6. 10. 11. Many examinees find the format of multiple correct answers for a single item frustrating. As they prepare to take the test, it would be helpful to tell them that this exam is probably unlike any other they have taken in two ways: a. They will be asked to observe a filmed encounter and answer multiple choice questions on what they felt the person would really say to her/himself at the 32d of the scene. Emphasize they are to give only ppg_response per item. b. For evegy item, more than one_pf_the answers ip correct. Credit will be given for any correct responses. A few scenes are very difficult because of their brevity. With any scene shorter than 30 seconds, you should warn them of its brevity prior to starting the projector. In any case where the sound is of marginal quality, read the dialogue aloud from the examiner's copy which has a complete typescript of the dialogue. If you have some question of the examinee's reading speed as a group (i.e., below the level expected of a college student), you may wish to lengthen the time per item to g9 seconds from 30. Please indicate you have done so in any correspondence on this data with us. Do pp£_lengthen the time for any group that has high reading capacities, since they tend to "obsess" over the responses rather than give their first impressions. You may wish to take a break during the test or give the test in two sessions. Our findings so far indicate this makes little difference in their scores, as long as the examinees cannot go back and change ppy_responses after the break on items completed previously. If you do take a break, it would be best to do so between two different scenes, rather than between different parts of the same scene. 0n the answer sheet, make sure the examinees put their name, sex, the date of the testing and the form of the exam taken (D or E). Have examinees use a soft lead pencil. 156 12. There are some scenes where there are more than two items. Be sure to allot sufficient time for completion. Alert the exams inees so they will not neglect answering additional items, or additional alternatives which may be on subsequent pages. 13. Post-test instructions. For examinees who have gone through IPR training, or have seen any of the Kagan IPR materials, the follow- ing suggestions are made to the examinees: Don't 5 ynpg recall what ygu saw previously during trainipg .pp determine how pp respond. If a particular scene looks famil- iar, stay with your immediate reaction to it. This is a 'sensitivity' not a 'memory' test. For most of us, our memory of past feelings is not as accurate as our capacity to recog- nize what's going on at the moment. INSTRUCTION - FORM D & E AFFECTIVE SENSITIVITY SCALE Forms D & E Instruction‘£p_Examinee You will be viewing short scenes of actual encounters between two or more individuals. You are to identify what feelings the people have toward themselves and toward the person they are working with. You will be instructed just prior to each scene which individuals to fo- cus on. Although in any one scene a client or interviewer may exhibit 3 var- iety of feelings, for the purpose of this instrument you are to concen— trate on identifying each person's last feelings in the scene. After you view each scene, you are to read the items and ask youself the following questions, depending on the specific instructions for the scene: If the people involved were to view this same scene, and if they were completely open and honest with themselves, (i.e., if they could identify their real feelings) which of these responses would they use to describe their feelings? After you decide which response accurately describes what the people are actually feeling whether about themselves or the others they are with, indicate your choice on the answer sheet. 0n the following pages are multiple choice items usually consisting of five responses each. Most scenes have two items, but a few have one or three items. Note the information you will be given: a) the context of what will be happening. This can include seating positions. b) the opening statement of the scene you will see, c) the length of time the scene 157 will be on the screen and d) the closing statement of the scene. In addition, the participants in each scene will be identified. Here is a Sample Item (minus the film segment) Scene AX TEACHER - STUDENTS a) Class discussing vocational planning b) Opening Statement (Teacher): "Is there anyone who knows the answer?" Time: 18 seconds After you have viewed Scene AX, you would turn the page and be given the following information: Scene AX TEACHER - STUDENTS (con't) d) Closing Statement (Teacher): "You look pretty upset." Item 1. What do you think the student is really saying to himself at this point? a. This exploring of my feelings is good. It makes me feel good. b. I feel very sad and unhappy. c. I'm groping and confused; I can't bring it all together. d. I wish he hadn't said that. It makes me angry. e. Upset! You don't know the half of it! After you have viewed Scene AX, you would turn the page and read the statements (Item 1, above) and would then decide which one best states what the client and/or interviewer would say about their own feelings after viewing the same scene. For example, if you decide "b" best states what the student is feeling, you would then find the "b" on your answer sheet and darken in the space for "b" (pencil only, please) Remember you are to concentrate on the latter part of each scene for determining your response. After you view the appropriate scene, you will have thirty seconds to answer each item. 158 The items on this test may seem very difficult. While there is a preferred response, there are other alternatives which are also scored as correct on each item. CONFIDENTIALITY. All of the participants in the scenes have given their informed consent as to the purpose and use of this material. However, because of the nature of the material discussed and their rights as human beings, we must ask you to treat all of the material as if the person told it to you personally and privately. INSTRUCTIONS FOR FEEDBACK It is our impressions that the most effective use of this scale is (l) as a feedback instrument, with opportunities following to work on selected areas, and (2) as a measure of change following a train- ing program. For the purpose of feedback, alloting a half-hour for individual feedback and discussion has been the most valuable method. Group feedback, however, does provide opportunities to clarify the meaning of the scores but does not readily permit us to help an individual examine the meaning of the feedback to that person. As mentioned previously, the skill of accurately reading another person is considered an important component of accurate empathy and effective interpersonal skills. The Affective Sensitivity Scale can provide the examinees with feedback about their differential sensi— tivity to a variety of context and types of individuals. For example, examinee "A" may find herself highly sensitive when the setting was a counseling setting but not so when the setting was more informal. She may find she is more aware of what is going on in children and women than in men. She may also discover that she is equally sensi- tive to the interviewer and client. This feedback could be useful in helping examinee "A" focus on the areas in which she has difficulty pp focus only on working in the settings or with the people where she is already effective. Definition & Explanation of Categories Current category scoring We are currently using a series of subscales which are based on the type of scenes we've used in scale development. Following is a description of each of these categories which are shown on the Pro- file Sheet in Figure l. A. ROLE-related items. For those scenes where there is a clearly identifiable interviewer and interviewee, we have compiled scores into the following two cateogories: Client and Interviewer. (Not included in this category are situations where there is no client, i.e., the health care team, the classroom situation, the informal discussion between friends.) I.aIll.I»\ -.I I‘uiol \I.lul|ll¢lnlulllul|..| IIIIIII lllllllll u d 1...... - \..-..0~z BNMHZOU \UZHHHmm MNHm .595 Nmm mu< . maom Illlllllllll'l 159 Figure 1. c: \O on 'II'IIIIII'IIIIIII o.» Illllllll'llllllllllll IIIIL on ll'll'l'llllll‘lllll" lllll oh lllllllllllllll'lllllllll'lllIIII Illll om OS on 00 oh 3 a O I H H S .a . O V I O o .2 m m m. s m. m m. m m m m. m n u m. s o n m p .m I a n n. a m d o o m u. .4 a u 3 K. _ I I. I. . I O o a B u M 1 e a muon— a T. 1 «SOHO magnum EH>HHHmzmm EHHUMMM< . mwuamvuzmamz 160 Client Sensitivity: these items involve any situation where there is a clearly definable client/patient. It includes only those ques- tions which ask the examinee to identify what the client/patient is experiencing at a particular time. We assume that this category assesses sensitivity to individuals who are in subordinate positions, or where the role definition ascribes them the role of the person seeking help or being interviewed. Interviewer Sensitivity: these items involve any situation where there is a clearly definable interviewer. These items occur in the same situations as the client sensitivity items. We assume that this category assesses sensitivity to individuals who are playing a dominant, seeking or clarifying role with others. In. the pre-post studies using IPR, we frequently find this to be one of the areas of greater change. Our assumption is that this area may also most closely reflect change in level of self-awareness, which is a basic intent of IPR training. AGE. We've also separated items based on the age of the person be- ing observed. Child Sensitivity: this scale includes all items where the examinee is asked how a child (under 18 yrs.) is reacting. Since these situations used to portray children are largely educa- tional and of the high age level, we are hesitant to generalize about sensitivity to children in other settings. For example, we find many professional parents (meaning parenthood is the chief pro- fession) scoring in midrange, despite evidence of sensitivity from other sources. Also, sensitivity to childhood psychopathology is not measured. We've found that usual IPR training shows no improve- ment pre to post on this scale, largely we assume, because IPR rarely involves children as clients or partners in the training. Adult Sensitivity: includes all items not scored in the Child area (53 items). .This category is probably the most reliable and general- izeable of the categories because of its size and the breadth of individuals used as stimuli. SEX. Items have also been categorized in terms of sensitivity to males and to females. Male Sensitivity: All items in which the examinee is to respond to a male adult or male child. We've discovered, in interviewing people who have scored high on male sensitivity, that this is frequently due to one or more of sev- eral factors: a) that such sensitivity, developmentally, had "sur- vival value" for the individual. That is, they needed to be aware as children, what father or some other dominant male in their lives was thinking or feeling in order to avoid punishment, rejection, 161 etc. b) Another reason offered was there was little anxiety in the interactions with dominant male figures in their current lives. c) There are also some individuals who are generally sensitive who score high on both male and female sensitivity, who are able to get beyond sex role stereotyping issues and see the more generic problems of men and women. Female Sensitivity: All items in which the examinee is to respond to a female adult or female child. The same factors influencing high male sensitivity also apply to female. However, for both male and female sensitivity 12! scores, we've also found multiple possible reasons: a) non-awareness of what is going on with a person of that sex, based on lack of contact in any depth with members of that sex. b) the use of defensive strategies developmentally which emphasized denial, projection or making general assumptions about the way individuals of that sex react. c) there are some individuals who do not score high even though there is external evidence of sensitivity and openness. Our hypothesis is that individuals who grow up in, or currently live in situations where affect is openly expressed and where each person takes responsibility for their feelings, there may be little need for being highly sensitive to subtle cues for what is going on. We think there may be some who do not score high on the test gener- ally because they aren't motivated by their personal or profession- al life circumstances to see such sensitivity as necessary. GROUP SIZE. Another way we have categorized the items is in terms of the size of the group being observed. Dyadic: Settings which involved only two people interacting. These scenes provide relatively greater opportunity to focus on the one or two individuals who are being observed. Group Settigg: Items where more than two people were interacting (classroom, health team, family). We've found some individuals who are generally sensitive, but cannot handle more than dyadic inter- actions. Hence, their profile shows generally high scores except in this category. We've also observed instances in which the re- verse takes place. This may be correlated with experience in group settings. Again, one must be careful not to generalize from these observations to all groups. It's also possible that the difference between scores on dyadic and group settings may reflect differential perceptual capacities for focusing and scanning. SETTING/CONTEXT: The remaining categories are based on all items from a particular setting. Educational settigg: Items included here are from classroom, teach- er—student interactions, interactions between counselors, principals, educational administrators. People who score high on this scale 162 tend to have more experience in this setting and are more sensitive to the issues and problems in secondary education in particular. It's difficult to generalize to other levels of education, because most of the items are from primary/middle school/secondary educa- tional settings. Some individuals who score low on this scale have reported difficult experiences in their education, and one or more of the scenes triggered unpleasant associations for them. Health Care Setting: Items from doctor-patient interactions and health team interactions. High scores tend to be more familiar with health care settings and the issues involved. (We've found that this doesn't necessarily apply to first year medical students, who may or may not have had previous experience in a health care setting.) Some people who score low describe past negative exper- -iences with health care personnel which are triggered by the scenes. Some people relied heavily on stereotypes of what doctors, nurses, etc. are like to choose their responses. Informal Setting: Items from the "Friends" interaction. Since there is no defineable interviewer or client, or expected role for either to play, we see this as sensitivity to more intimate, infor- mal relations. One major handicap of this scale is size: there are only a few items and only one setting. However, our impression is that people who prefer informality in their interactions with others tend to score higher than those who prefer more clearly defined roles in their interactions with others. Hence, sometimes we find someone who is very high on psychotherapy setting, but low in in- formal, or vice-verse. ProblemrSolvingySetting: Items where there is a clearly defined issue being discussed between an interviewer/counselor and a client. This includes discussing death and dying, marital problems, identity problems (e.g. becoming a doctor), academic performance, etc. It does ppt include medical problems or what we'll subsequently discuss as "psychotherapeutic" or counseling issues. We've found people scoring high who've had experience in crisis coun- seling settings and thbse who see themselves and are seen by others as effective problem solvers: peOple who can clearly define an issue and see alternative ways to approach it. Low scorers sometimes are the result of one or more of the issues being discussed reflecting one of their personal concerns which influenced reactions to the scene. For example, several people described difficulty with the death and dying items because there had been a recent or unresolved death in their family. Counseling & Psychotherapy Setting: Items where the iesue is not clearly defined; where the interaction is between a therapist and client, or therapist and family; and where the purpose of the inter- action is not one of solving a particular problem (at least not immediately) but more one of clarifying feelings, assumptions over a period of time to allow for personal growth and where the client appears to be deeply troubled. Those who do well on these items .163 SCORING PREF- ACCEP- STANDARD Category # ITEMS ERRED TABLE RANDOM TOTAL SCORE Client 28 Interviewer 26 Adult 53 Child 10 Male 36 Female 27 Group 13 Dyad 49 Educational 18 Health Care 11 Informal 6 Counseling 9 Psychotherapy l9 o-h F.\1 \ .TL oh. 164 tend to be those who've had experience in these settings (on either side of the couch) and/or who are sensitive to basic issues which lie beyond the presenting concerns of the client. Low scorers may in- clude individuals who've had negative experiences in psychotherapy and bring these with them to their responses, or who find their reac- tions triggered by the scenes they've witnessed. For example, several people have had intense reactions to the discussion around divorce and the children's reactions to the extent they couldn't be objective about what they witnessed. Total Score: All items summed together. We're not sure what the total score means at this point, since we feel the variety of scenes, situations etc. and the diversity of response to them suggests to us that much of perceptual empathy is situation specific or dependent on past experiences with a particular kind of individual. 0n pre to post training, we've found some individuals becoming generally more sensi- tive, which shows up on the total score, while others have shown no total score change, but who've focused on specific areas and have , shown improvement there. Additionally we've found that those who score high in the test tend to suspend judgment or interpretation of what they have just witnessed, while low scorers are generally in- clined to make assumptions or judgments about what they have witnessed. (see Schneider, Kagan & Werner, 1977; Kagan, Schneider & Werner, 1977 for further details.) T’” Generaliziability of results We do not yet know to what extent the perceptual skill measured by the A.S.S. is generalizable to the other interpersonal skills. It has been found to accurately identify individuals with low capacity for empathy. It has also proved sensitive to changes that take place in individuals as a function of training. It has correlated with measures of commun- icating skills in an interview setting in some studies. Further re- search is necessary before we can claim much beyond this. Not Measured by the Affective Sensitivity Scale This test is not intended to be an adequate measure of the following components of accurate empathy: Self awareness. This capacity of individuals to be aware of how a situation or another person is affecting them on an intellectual, emo- tional or physiological level is assumed to be an important factor in being able to identify with others; to provide others with feedback about'their impact on others (self-disclosure) , to differentiate own feelings from those of others. Self-awareness is assumed basic to these aspects of empathy. Because of various technical and logistical limitations, this version of the A.S.S. does not measure this skill ex- cept indirectly; i.e., one explanation for a lpg_score on the A.S.S. is low self awareness, which prevents the examinee from being able to iden- tify with or differentiate themselves from the individual in the film vignettes. ., . f 3:. z. .. . - .-- f. nix; 6L :- . ,. 1...: x o. .v- HE D "I“. 0 ti“ .7 .7. Ta. 5.. .3 I «U P. «3 v a‘ «r».— ..a .5 ..- «is «4 2. xi :4 .9» at 4,. .(J .C .3 a. a: b; . a .x. .m- 165 Response skills. Another component of accurate empathy not tapped by this instrument is the communication skills necessary to convey to another that they are aware of what the other person is experiencing. These skills can involve (1) basic listening skills, i.e., that the person wants to and is interested in hearing what the other is saying, (2) interpretive skills, which convey that the person understands what the other is saying and, (3) facilitative skills, which convey that the person is willing and able to help the other listen to themselves and understand and accept their own behavior. Again, the A.S.S. does 225 measure these skills. Memory/continuity factors. Because the examinee is asked to respond only to a series of very brief excerpts from various interviews, the A.S.S. cannot assess their ability to associate previous knowledge of a person with their current presenting concern, nor can it assess their capacity to understand the person's difficulty in depth. Interactional skills. In addition to the capacity to communicate em- pathetic understanding to another person, there are the skills associ- ated with establishing an effective working relationship, facilitating the emergence of data, client education and the generation and testing of hypothesis in an interview. This would require the examinees to engage in an actual interview, report their behavior following the session and be rated on all these behaViors. Individual Feedback Face validity. This has been our experience so far: 1. People generally felt there was a high correlation between their test results vis a vis females, males and children, and their own perceptions of their life experiences with these three groups. 2. When looking at patterns of scoring (number of +2 responses versus number of +1 responses), students seemed to feel that these numbers accurately reflected their general style. That is, people who had mostly +1 responses characterized themselves as conservative in making judgments about people--they say they always wait for more information. On the other hand, people who had more +2 responses tended to report that in general they judge people rather quickly, frequently relying on "intuitive" feelings. 3. People did better on items from familiar settings. People who had done counseling, for example, scored high on counseling and psychother- apy. Most people felt that the "setting" scores accurately reflected their past experiences. Feedback Session. In general, we have found that the best way to go through a feedback session is as follows: 1. Explain the chart to the examinee. The vertical lines divide the scores into categories. Then explain each category at the top of the chart, using the section, "Definition and Explanation of Categories" 166 as a guideline. Explain how the socres are standardized and what the standard deviations are (x - 50, S.D. - 10). 2. Look at the person's individual scores. We usually focus on the better scores first. Ask if the examinee felt the pattern of scoring was reflective of his or her own perceptions of himself or herself, i.e. their highs and lows i£_more than a S.D. apart. It is useful in pro- ceeding from category to category to try to remind the person of which film clips were included in that category. 3. It is especially useful to explain how the raw scores were derived on the backs of the feedback sheet, so they are aware of the way they have scored, and the size of the category in terms of reliability. We feel that it is very useful to allow a half-hour/subject for the feedback. Many people have made admissions to us (and for some of them, for the first time to themselves) of feelings of deficiencies in certain interactions and ability to relate to certain people. There was a great need to talk this out and most of them asked for suggestions for improve- ment. We're reluctant to state that the test showed or that we felt they had a problem. We let that come from them. We also never suggest ways one might improve interactional skills unless it is specifically requested. Some people who did not "do well" (in their opinion) on the test seemed to need to defend that. We accept their defenses and agree that whatever they say is certainly possible, while encouraging them to use the test for whatever stimulus value it has. Depending on the "contract" with people who take this test, it seems only fair to offer people private feedback (or a chance to talk pri- vately with someone about it if explanation of the scoring is done in a group). For people enrolled in IPR courses, for example, this might not be necessary, but for a class of students or another group of people who do not have a way to improve their skills formally, then individual feedback is desirable. People who are going to undertake the job of providing feedback should be thoroughly familiar with the test, the scoring, methods of feedback, etc. They certaily should be people who have the capability of dealing with some rather heavy life issues which may be revealed. APPENDIX C SCALES AND SUBSCALES, ITEM CONTENT 1(57 m n v v v v v v v v v m m m m m m m m m m m m m m Huuoa amouosuoso>nm ma x x x x x x x x x x x x unwaonnsoo NH x x x x x x x x x x x HMEMOHcH Ha cuaumm ed demoduoosom m x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x come a x x x x x 95.5 H. x x x x x x x x x x x x madame m x x x. x x x x x x x x x x can: m x o««so v x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x venom m x x x x x x x nosma>umusn N x x x x x x x x x unmanu A vn an an an on an mu on ow m~ va mN «N H~ ow ma ma no on ma va ma NH HA ca ‘ EouH oasomnsm «— O z 2 A M n. H a h m 980m a h moanwmm a Buom - mason hua>auansom o>ouoouu¢ .mwsncoauuamm oasomnam - amuH 168 m m m m m m m m m m m m m m m m m m m m m m m m m m m m Houoa aH x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x hmuuonuonu>mm nH a msHHmncsoo ~H w HmEMOMGH HH HH nuHuom OH «H x x x x x x x x x x HucoHusUsom a av x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x come a vH x x x x x x x x macaw 5 ha x x x x x x x x mHquh w on x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x OHM! m OH x x x x x x oHHsu v mm x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x uHso< m on x x x x x x x x x x x x x uoon>umusn N an x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x ucoHHU H wonaaz no no He ow mm an pm mm mm cm mm ~m Hm on me av so we we vv nv we Hv ov mm mm on mm mm . EmuH mHsounom on 00 mm ‘4 N N x 3 > D m m 0 ocean «H «H HH oH moooHou HomocHucouv a soup I oHsom auH>Hancom o>Huomuu4 .mHsmsoHuHuom oHuounsw I Elan Empirical Indicies 169 Item Choice b C \DmxlmU'l-hUNI-H 3,4 MPH - NH HHm MU'IH N NH |-‘ HUI UlN l-‘l-‘NU'I gh- WNNW 1,2 NLn era N ‘ NN HfiHN ‘ N N s U s uh NI—‘NN-hNNN N 1,2 Empirical Indicies 170 Choice I tem a b c d e 50 l 1 51 l 1,2 52 1,2 3 53 5 54 1,2 55 5 2 56 5 4 57 2 2 58 1 59 5 5 60 61 1,2 2 2 62 5 63 2 JKey l = Suspiciousness 2 = Discounting 3 = Polyanna 4 = Feeling Denial 5 = Feeling Awareness Empirical Subscales Subscale r-I u s 'o 'o c 44 m m Item '7 3 E a .c m m m 0‘ O- 'H m «4 E G .0 m H H o l x 2 3 x 4 x 5 x 6 x x 7 x x 171 Empirical Subscales (Continued) Subscale .- 3 o o c 44 m m H +1 u > Item 5 3 E 3 pi o. «4 m -H E s .Q B: H H O 8 x 9 x x 10 x x 11 x 12 x x 13 x x 14 x x 15 x x 16 x 17 x x 18 x x x 19 x x 20 x x 21 22 x 23 x 24 x x 25 x x 26 x 27 x x 28 x 29 x x 30 X x 31 x x 32 x 33 x x 34 X x 35 x 36 x 37 X x 38 X x 39 x x x 40 X x x 41 X x 42 x 43 x x 44 45 X X x 46 x x 47 x x 48 x x 172 Empirical Subscales (Continued) . _4 Subscale u .'3 13 c G 'H o 4) E H +J H > m o H H u .c m m m H O) D- 'H m -H E c .0 m F! +4 0 49 x x 50 x x 51 x x x 52 x x x 53 x x 54 X 55 x x x 56 x x x 57 x 58 x x 59 x x 60 X x x 61 x x 62 x x 63 x x x APPENDIX D AFFECTIVE SENSITIVITY SCALE (Fomnrn Reproduced here with the permission of the authors AFFECTIVE SENSITIVITY SCALE Form D @ COpyright Normal 1. Kagan, Ph.D. John M. Schneider, Ph.D. 1975 All rights reserved 6/75 173 174 AFFECTIVE SENSITIVITY SCALE Forms D & E Instructions You will be viewing short scenes of actual encounters between two or more individuals. You are to identify what feelings the people have toward themselves and toward the person they are working with. You will be instructed just prior to each scene which individua1(s) to focus on. Although in any one scene a client or interviewer may exhibit a variety of feelings, for the purpose of this instrument you are to concentrate on identifying his last feelings in the scene. In some scenes, you will also be asked about the nature of the last response. After you view each scene, you are to read the items and ask yourself the following questions, depending on the specific instruction for the scene: If the people involved were to view this same scene, and if they were completely open and honest with themselves, (i.e., if they could identify their real feelings) which of these responses would they use to describe their feelings? After you decide which response accurately describes what the people are actually feeling whether about themselves or the others they are with, indicate your choice on the answer sheet. On the following pages are multiple choice items usually consisting of five responses each. Most scenes have two items, but a few have one or three items. Note the information you will be given: a) the context of what will be happening. This can include seating positions. b) the opening state- ment of the scene you will see, c) the length of time the scene will be on the screen and d) the closing statement of the scene. In addition, the participants in each scene will be identified. Here Is A Sample Item Scene AX TEACHER - STUDENTS a) +Class discussion vocational planning b) +Opening Statement (Teacher): "Is there anyone who knows the answer?" Time: 18 Seconds +'c) 175 After you have viewed Scene AX, you would turn the page and be given the following information. Scene AX TEACHER - STUDENTS (con't.) d) +Closing Statement (Teacher): "You look pretty upset." Item 1. What do you think the student is really saying to himself at this point? a. This exploring of my feelings is good. It makes me feel good. *b. I feel very sad and unhappy. c. I'm groping and confused; I can't bring it all together. d. I wish he hadn't said that. It makes me angry. e. Upset. You don't know the half of it! After you had viewed Scene AX, you would read these five statements (Item 1) and would then decide which one best states what the client and/or interviewer would say about their own feelings after viewing the same scene. For example, if you decide "b" best states what the student is feeling, you would then find the "b" on your answer sheet and darken in the space for "b". Remember you are to concentrate on the latter part of each scene in determining the most accurate description of the person's feelings or response to same. After you view the appropriate scenes, you will have thirty seconds to answer each item. Generally there will be two or three items per scene, so the total time will be 60 or 90 seconds for answering the items. The items on this test may seem very difficult. While there is a pre- ferred response, there are several alternatives which can also be cor- rect. These alternatives will also be included in the scoring. CONFIDENTIALITY All of the participants in the following scenes have given their informed consent as to the purpose and use of this material. However, because of the nature of the material discussed and their rights as human beings, we must ask to treat all of the material as if the per- son told it to you personally and privately. 176 Affective Sensitivity Scale Form D PLEASE DO NOT MARK 0N ANSWER.BOOKLET Scene A FORMER TEACHER AND 4TH GRADE STUDENT Setting: Woman and 9 year old girl sitting in chairs. Opening statement (teacher): "I heard you made a movie about your classroom." Time: 49 Seconds DO NOT TURN PAGE UNTIL THE SCENE IS OVER .177 Scene A Closing statement (student): "I did it Tuesday." Item.l. What do you think the little girl is really saying to herself at this point? a. Boy, am I glad Ms. S. is here. I'd really be nervous otherwise. b. Why is she asking me all those questions? She already knows the answers. c. I hope I don't get stuck and not know what to say. d. I'm really scared. Maybe she'll ask me some- thing really tough. e. Boy, those lights sure are hot. Item 2. What do you think the teacher is really saying to herself at this point? a. I'm really nervous. What if Betsy won't talk? b. Why am I asking her questions that I already know the answers to? c. I wonder what she is feeling about my asking such dumb questions? d. How can I make that Betsy shows what she really can do? e. I wish I didn't know so much about Betsy already - it makes it hard to be spontaneous. 178 __' _'AALIL.JH_L _|__~_;'_LA L 1'0‘ ' v ,. .., .II Scene B TEACHERPSTUDENT (con't.) Setting: Same Opening Statement (teacher): "How did you feel about it?" Time: 43 Seconds .‘o 44. .AJ_4 DO NOT TURN PAGE UNTIL THE SCENE IS OVER 179 Scene B Closing Statement (teacher): "You were afraid you wouldn't be accepted." Item 3. What do you think the student is really saying to herself at this point? a. Accepted. What does she mean by that? b. Boy, I was really scared then. c. I'm.aot sure I really want to tell her. d. I wish she wouldn't ask me so many questions. It's hard to think when she does that. e. This is boring. I already told her all about this. Item 4. What do you think the teacher is really saying to herself? a. Now why did I say that. Accepted is an adult way of saying it. b. Now she knows I'm really with her. c. Betsy never worries about being accepted. Why did I say that? d. I'm putting words in her mouth. I wonder what she's really feeling? e. She looks puzzled. Doesn't she know what "accepted" means? Scene C TEACHER-STUDENT (con't.) Setting: Same Opening Statement (teacher): "You say that people make the difference?" Time: 35 Seconds DO NOT TURN PAGE UNTIL THE SCENE IS OVER 180 Scene C Closing Statement: "Let's talk first of all how the classroom is different." Item 5. What do you think the student is really saying to herself at this point? a. Whew, I don't have to answer that one. b. Hey, I didn't get a chance to tell you what I thought. c. I'm getting confused. What does she want? d. She's just like all big people. Won't give you a chance to say anything. e. I'm getting tired of answering so many questions. Item 6. What do you think the teacher is really saying to herself at this point? a. I really wish she would sit still and pay atten- tion to my questions. b. I'm not feeling very effective in this situation. c. Oh, oh. She's not going to respond. I'd better change the topic. d. She's taking too much time. I've got to fill in the silence on the tape. e. That's too hard a concept for her. Maybe I can help her by switching topics. Scene D COUNSELOR-RAPE VICTIM Setting: Male counselor talking with woman rape victim Opening Statement (student): "I find him a disgust- ing person." Time: 25 Seconds DO NOT TURN PAGE UNTIL SCENE IS OVER 181 Scene D COUNSELOR-RAPE VICTIM (con't.) Closing Statement (Advisor): "What about him still scares you?" Item 7. At this point, what do you think the advisor was really saying to himself. a. She's really feeling sorry for herself. b. Disgusting. Where does that come from? c. She looks confused now. It's not clear what the conflict is for her. d. I want to help her deal with her feelings. e. Frightening and disgusting - confused, too, it looks like. Item 8. At this point, what do you think the student was really saying to herself. a. Scared. If you only knew! b. Gee, you really know what I am feeling. Right on! c. I'm not going to say anymore until I know if he is judging me or not. d. I'm not scared. I'm so angry I can't even think straight. e. I'm scared of all men now. Scene E COUNSELOR-RAPE VICTIM Setting: Same Opening Statement (Student): "Everything about the incident was so totally unpredictable to me." Time: 30 Seconds DO NOT TURN PAGE UNTIL THE SCENE IS OVER Scene E Item 9. Item 10. Item 11. 182 Closing Statement (Counselor): "What are your nightmares?" What do you think the interviewer was really saying to himself? I want her to explore her fears in depth. Her dream content will tell me what she really is feeling. I'm getting bored with what she is saying and want to change the subject. VI'm frightened about unpredictable people, too, so I can't help her with that. Maybe if I can distract her from that. There is a whole mess of stuff she hasn't dealt with. I wonder how to get her into it. What do you think the client is really saying to herself at this point? a. b. I feel frustrated that he doesn't respond to my concerns about the rapist's unpredictability. The whole incident is so hard for me to under- stand - it was so unpredictable - I wonder if it will happen again? I'd like to get my trust/faith in human nature back again. Nightmares! God, if he only knew! He looks concerned, but is he - really? What did the client want to hear at this point? His unpredictability . . . that's what really makes you afraid of him. And now you're not sure you can trust others? What are your nightmares . . . when you see him in the halls, what are you afraid will happen? Irrational . . . fantastic . . . not normal,are you afraid you are going crazy? .183 Scene E Item 10. (con't.) d. I don't think you are irrational or not normal for reacting that way. Scene F TERMINALLY ILL MAN - INTERVIEWER Opening Statement (man): "Three people in that group - died the first week." o 4L Time:7 1 minute 39 Seconds DO NOT TURN PAGE UNTIL SCENE IS OVER 184 Closing Statement (man): "So we had grown rather close . . . ah . . . ah . . . ah" At this point, what do you think the terminally ill man is really saying to himself? My god, it's hard to believe that he is really I'm really very lucky that it happened to him I really don't want to talk about my feelings about Tom, it's still too painful for me to I really am glad I had a chance to get to know Tom. I feel richer for the opportunity. Why did he have to die? Dammit, it's just not What is the interviewer really saying to himself? He doesn't show much of his feelings. You look like you could cry right now, Bob. I didn't realize how close the two of you had Can sometimes . . . could sometimes. Sounds like he's still denying that Tom has died. He's not telling me his feelings - he's talk- ing about Tom and him. Must be it's too painful *- -‘ TERMINALLY ILL MAN a INTERVIEWER Opening Statement (Terminally ill man): "Being occupied with life is a way to make the fear of death go away." Scene F Item 12. a. gone. b. and not me. c. acknowledge. d. e. fair. Item 13. a. b. It's okay. c. grown. d. e. still. Scene C Time: 63 Seconds Do NOT TURN PAGE UNTIL SCENE IS OVER Item 14. Item 15. 185 Closing Statement (Interviewer): "You really cher- ish those times." (Ill man): "Yeah." What do you think the interviewer is really saying to himself? a. To be able to cherish those times makes it worth living. I can understand that. b. But what about your anger, your fears? c. That makes me uncomfortable. I really squander those times myself. I don't want to deal with that. d. It still sounds like he is denying and escaping into his work. e. I wonder if he feels like I'm pushing him too hard. What do you think the terminally ill man really ex- perienced in response to the group leader saying "you really cherish those times." a. It sounds like he understands, but how can he - he isn't terminal. b. Cherish . . . that's the word. c. He understands how important those things are - he must appreciate living, too. d. He buys it. I wish I was half as convinced myself. e. It makes me happy just to think about it. 186 Scene H FEMALE PATIENT - MALE SURGEON Post-operative interview (one month) Opening Statement (Surgeon): "You had your operation for peritonitis?" Time: 37 Seconds DO NOT TURN PAGE UNTIL SCENE IS OVER 187 Scene H FEMALE PATIENT - MALE SURGEON (con't.) Closing Statement (Surgeon): "You don't remember too much about that?" (Patient): "No." Item 16. What do you think the surgeon is really saying to himself at this point? a. She doesn't remember that! She sure screamed loud enough at the time. b. I didn't think she was conscious, but sometimes you can't be sure. c. I guess I was so busy with the technical details I didn't even notice if she was awake or not. d. Her smile tells me she does remember - but it's too painful to recall. e. I'm glad she doesn't remember much about that. Item 17. What is the patient really saying to herself at this point? a. I may be smiling, but my memory of that whole thing is a nightmare! b. Glad - you better believe it! c. You know damn well, doctor, that I was uncon- scious at the time. d. That was scarey. I don't want to remember. e. It makes me uncomfortable to think about it. Scene I PATIENT - SURGEON (con't.) Opening Statement (Surgeon): "Do you remember how desperate you were feeling?" Time: 1 minute 30 Seconds DO NOT TURN PAGE UNTIL SCENE IS OVER .188 Scene I PATIENT - SURGEON (con't.) Closing Statement (Patient): "It's exciting just to know I can smile, now." Item 18. What do you think the patient is really saying to herself at this point? a. It's hard to believe it was really me going through all that. b. He can't really understand what it's like to lose a year and a half of your life that way. c. I really appreciate even the little things now. It's such a relief. d. I wish I could convince him that I really feel much better than I have for a long time. e. I'm not convinced, really, yet - but I'm work- ing hard at convincing you I'm happy and not depressed anymore. Item 19. What do you think the surgeon is really saying to himself at this point? a. I wish I could believe what you are saying, but I can't. b. It's a relief for me to see you smile, too. c. I must be careful not to get too optimistic. She still could have a relapse. d. Exciting, yes, but it still won't be easy to re-create your life after the past year and a half. e. NOw comes the tough part - she's got to find meaning in her life now that the pain is gone. Scene J 189 Time: 58 Seconds DOCTOR - PATIENT Opening Statement (Doctor): "And the childbirth, pregnancy." DO NOT TURN PAGE UNTIL SCENE IS OVER 190 Scene J DOCTOR - PATIENT (con't.) Closing Statement (Patient): "No one ever did explain to me." Item 20. What do you think the patient is really saying to herself at this point? a. I'm still a little confused about it. b. Nobody helps. NOBODY HELPS. Are you going to join the "shit list" of nobody helps? c. Even now, it really annoys me to think that he really didn't take the time to explain it to me. d. He was so busy. I really accept that. I wish he could have explained it though. e. I still resent not knowing why for sure. Are you going to be the same way? Item 21. What do you think the doctor was saying to himself at this point? ' a. She's saying it calmly enough, but it sounds like she's still upset about it. b. She expects a lot . . . and she complains a lot. c. I'd better be careful not to get caught on the same Spot. I'd better explain it to her. d. I'm really uncomfortable with her attacking an- other physician. Let's go on . . . I need more information. e. Is it just information she wants? What else? Scene K DOCTOR - PATIENT (con't.) Opening Statement (Patient): "Like for another baby . . to have another." Time: 50 Seconds DO NOT TURN PAGE UNTIL THE SCENE IS OVER Scene X Item 22. Item 23. 191 DOCTOR - PATIENT (con't.) Closing Statement (Doctor): "So there are several checks and balances." What do you think the doctor is saying to himself at this point? a. b. I can understand your concern, but we really do know what we are doing. I'd better be straight with her. She doesn't seem like someone who is going to back down and just be a good patient. I don't really want to get involved in this right now. I hope that is what she is asking, but I'm not really sure. Could she be concerned about some- thing else? I'd better remind my nurse to give her that pregnancy handout. What do you think the patient is saying at this point? a. b. That's helpful. I didn't know they could do that. I feel better. But that's not what I'm worried about. I'm really scared that nobody will tell me anything when I really need to know. But what are the risks? I trust him. He is really being straight with me. But aren't x-rays dangerous too? Do you really know what you're saying? 192 Scene L HEALTH CARE TEAM Setting: Physician Nurse Social Worker Physician (male) (female) (female) (male) Opening Statement (Dr. on right): "About a week after the baby was born . . ." Time: 59 Seconds DO NOT TURN PAGE UNTIL THE SCENE IS OVER .193 Scene L HEALTH CARE TEAM (con't.) Closing Statement (Dr. on left: "I . . . I . . don't remember exactly." Item 24. What do you think the doctor on the left (Dan) was saying to himself at this point? a. You son of a bitch! Putting pg on the spot like that. b. I'm really embarrassed. I should know, but I'm blank. c. Where did that come from? Is he playing games with me? d. Oh my God. What if some physicians see this? They'll really think I'm a dummy. e. He caught me not paying attention. Item 25. What do you think the doctor on the right was saying to himself at this point? a. Dummy! Every doctor knows what Stevens-Johnson's syndrome is. b. Oh, oh. I've put him on the spot. He's gonna get me for that. c. I shouldn't have done that. I really embar- rassed him. d. Gee, I hoped Dan could answer that. After all, we are the physicians here. e. Well, I guess I'll have to take charge again. Nobody else really seems capable of doing any- thing else. Item 26. What do you think the social worker (who asked what Stevens-Johnson's syndrome was) is saying to herself at this point? a. He's passing the buck. Will anybody answer my question? b. I'd better be careful about showing what I don't know. Look what happened to Dan. (continued on next page) 194 c. WOw! That happened so fast I'm not sure what's going on between the two of them! d. He's; trying to exclude me by using those med- ical terms. e. What kind of game is he (physician on right) playing? He really put Dan on the spot. Scene M "FRIENDS" Opening Statement (Man): "What do you want to talk about?" Time: 8 Seconds DO NOT TURN PAGE UNTIL THE SCENE IS OVER 195 Scene M. "FRIENDS" (con't.) Closing Statement (Man): "That kinda puts me on the spot a little bit." Item 27. What do you think he's saying to himself at this point? a. That bitch! She's really making me look bad. b. Oh, oh, she caught me. How can I re-coup my error? c. I wish I could crawl away and hide. I'm really embarrassed. d. O.K., baby, don't you play games with me or I won't be so nice with you. e. She's right. We're in this together. But how do we start? Item 28. What do you think she's saying to herself at this point? a. He's not going to pass me the buck like he usually does. b. Oh, oh, I've put him on the spot. He'll get me for that. c. I usually let him do that, but not this time, I'm.not going to take responsibility for start- ing this. d. He's uncomfortable. How can I help him? e. He seems to be willing to share responsibility. Now to start. Scene N "FRIENDS" Opening Statement (Woman): "Say, I don't know . . . when I think about . . Time: 25 Seconds DO NOT TURN PAGE UNTIL THE SCENE IS OVER 196 Scene N "FRIENDS" (con't.) Closing Statement (Man): "No." Item 29. What do you think the man is saying to himself at this point? a. I hope she really understands that. She is more important than that to me. b. You may have a mind, baby, but you've got a body that just won't stop! c. If you really knew what I was thinking, I'd be in trouble. I've got to be careful. d.' That confuses me. Where did she get the idea that I saw her as a body. e. Sounds like she's got a hangup - she thinks all men are after her body. Item 30. What do you think the woman is saying to herself at this point? a. It's a relief to know he does feel that way. I trust him. b. He looks uncomfortable with that. Maybe he does think I'm only a body? c. You'd better feel that way, buster, or this is the last you'll see of me! d. I hope he sees me that way. Please say that I'm.nore than just a body to you. e. I've been burned so many times, it's not worth it to even get involved if that's the way he feels. Scene 0 "FRIENDS" (con't.) Opening Statement (Woman): "Know you're going to hear me." Time: 40 Seconds DO NOT TURN PAGE UNTIL THE SCENE IS OVER .197 Scene 0 "FRIENDS (con't.) Closing Statement (Woman): "Exactly." Item 31. What do you think the woman is saying to herself at this point? 8. b. Exactly - schmackly. I don't even understand what I just said! We really are being very tentative with each other. What are we afraid of? It sounds like we are trying to reassure each other. Maybe we aren't sure. I wonder if we are talking about the same thing? Maybe If I smile pretty, he'll change the sub- ject. Item 32. What do you think the man is saying to himself at this point? She sure is pretty when she smiles. We are being very superficial with each other. I wish I knew why. Exactly! How can she say that when we are being so vague? This is really hard for me to talk about. I'm really having trouble making the words come out. I feel comfortable about our relationship. Scene P CLASSROOM - COUNSELOR Opening Statement (Teacher - counselor): Time: 55 Seconds "Last time when we talked about career education. . ." DO NOT TURN PAGE UNTIL THE SCENE IS OVER 198 Scene P CLASSROOM - COUNSELOR Closing Statement (Teacher): "O.K., your best friend's deaf . . . great . . . great." Item 33. What do you think the teacher is saying to herself at this point? a. Wow, that's a heavy reason for choosing such a career. b. "Great." That's a dumb thing to say about her best friend's deafness. c. Well, I got her to talk. Now, who else can I get to respond? d. Oh, oh. What do I ask now? e. That's really not a very good reason for going into speech therapy, Linda, but I won't embar- rass you in front of the class. Item 34. What do you think Linda is saying to herself at this point? a. I'm confused. Does she really think it's great my best friend's deaf? b. She really isn't interested in me. She didn't really listen to what I said. c. I know what she means. She really is nice. d. She must really be embarrassed after saying some- thing like that. e. Oh, oh. She must really think that's a dumb reason. I'm so embarrassed. Scene 0 CLASSROOM Opening Statement (Teacher): "Can you tell us any- thing about how you came to that decision?" Time: 28 Seconds DO NOT TURN PAGE UNTIL THE SCENE IS OVER 199 CLASSROOM (con't.) Scene Q Closing Statement (Sherry): "Because . . . be- cause, like . . ." Item 35. What do you think Sherry is saying to herself at this point? a. She stole my idea. Now I'm.having to think of something different. b. I'm so embarrassed. I keep stumbling over these words. c. I forgot what I was going to say. My mind is a blank. d. I'm.ao nervous. Everybody is watching me. e. I want to make this come out good. If only she (teacher) will stay with me 'til I get it out. Item.36. What do you think the teacher is saying to herself at this point? a. Sherry had her answer stolen. She doesn't know what to say. b. The poor kid. She really trying but she just doesn't have what it takes. c. I'd be embarrassed too if I stumbled over those words. d. I wish I could get you out of the bind you're in, Sherry. e. Come on, get it out. We don't have much time. Scene R CLASSROOM Opening Statement (Teacher): "Is there anybody here - who hasn't thought very much about . . ." Time: 45 Seconds DO NOT TURN PAGE UNTIL THE SCENE IS OVER Scene R Item 37. Item 38. 200 CLASSROOM (con't.) Closing Statement (Teacher): "Is that what you're saying?" (Garrick): "Yeah." (Teacher): "O.K." What might Garrick be saying to himself at this point? 3. She doesn't really understand, but it's o.k. I won't push it. Whew, I'm glad I got all that out without stumbling over my words. She heard me. She's o.k. Well, that's just what I said. Why did she have to repeat me? I feel good. I opened my mouth and got a stroke for it. What might the teacher be saying to herself at this point? a. b. I'm really pleased I was able to hear him right and have him agree. It feels good. He really didn't say anything, but I made him feel good anyway. He doesn't speak often. I'm glad he did and I could respond. I didn't hear it all (I blanked for awhile). I'll try to get out of it, see if I can fake it. These kids are really being open. I'm enjoying this. 201 Scene S PRINCIPAL - JR. HIGH SCHOOL Opening Statement (Principal): "You were sent in here again today." Time: 28 Seconds DO NOT TURN PAGE UNTIL THE SCENE IS OVER 202 Scene S PRINCIPAL - JR. HIGH STUDENT (con't.) Closing Statement (Student): "Right." Item 39. What do you think the student is saying to him— self at this point? a. o.k., what's he going to lay on me this time? b. This guy really turns me off. c. I'm scared. What's he going to do to me? d. There is no way he is gonna pin this rap on me. I'll get out of it somehow. e. It's hopeless. Nobody understands or even wants to understand me. Why try? Item 40. What do you think the principal is saying to himself at this point? a. I must be firm. He'll take advantage of me if I'm not. b. I'm being a jerk and this kid sees right through it. c. I'm angry. I don't like to do this, but he keeps getting in trouble. d. The way he said "right." He's scared. I'm getting through to him. e. Well, at least he agrees with me about that. Scene T PRINCIPAL - STUDENT (con't.) Time: 43 Seconds Opening Statement (Student): "Like in career ed., I honestly think . . ." DO NOT TURN PAGE UNTIL THE SCENE IS OVER Scene I h“ 2033 Scene T PRINCIPAL - STUDENT (con't.) Closing Statement (Principal): "Well, you don't have to . . ." Item 41. What do you think the principal is saying to him- self at this point? a. He really doesn't understand the purpose of career education. I'll explain it to him. b. You little , you're putting me on the de- fensive again, like you always do. c. I'd better catch myself. I'm starting to de- fend the teacher, and he may have a valid point there. d. How can I get across to him what I want. I'm not doing it. e. At his age, he should know what he wants to do, or at least make a choice. He's just avoiding responsibility. Item 42. What do you think the student is saying to himself at this point? a. I've got him now, he's arguing with me. He can't win. b. He isn't responding to how I feel. c. It's really hard for me in career ed. I wish I could convince him of that. d. This is boring. When will it be over? e. No way he's gonna convince me to go back to that class. Scene U COUNSELOR - JR. HIGH STUDENT Opening Statement (Counselor): "You kinda wonder if you're gonna get the suspension." Time: 72 Seconds DO NOT TURN PAGE UNTIL SCENE IS OVER 204 Scene U COUNSELOR - JR. HIGH STUDENT (con't.) Closing Statement (Counselor): "You sound like you're a little bit concerned." Item 43. What might the student be saying to himself at this point? a. He can see right through me. That scares me. b. I'm gonna have to be more careful. He's getting to close. c. Not really. We had to talk about something, though. d. This is really a drag. I've heard all this before. e. He understands. He knows I'm worried. I can trust him. Item 44. What do you think the counselor is saying to him- self at this point? a. You and I both know we're talking about you, Jerry. It's hard for you to admit, though. b. He's uncomfortable. I've put him on the spot. What's he gonna do with that? c. He really doesn't know how else to ask for help. I hope I can get through to him. d. I guess I'm nervous too. I'd really like to help him, though. e. Oh, oh. I guess he's in more trouble than even I know about. Scene V THERAPIST - CLIENT Opening Statement (Client): "But with me, she has condemned me." Time: 40 Seconds DO NOT TURN PAGE UNTIL THE SCENE IS OVER 205 everything I do." sad at this point." Scene V THERAPIST - CLIENT (con't.) Closing Statement (Client): "She has to condemn Item 45. What do you think the client is saying to himself at this point? a. I wonder if he knows what I'm trying to say about my mother. b. It's not fair. Why should my brother get all her affection? It's not fair. c. I am so mad at my mother that I'm having trouble expressing how intense that feeling is. d. Don't you try and tell me it's all in my head buster, or I'll walk right out of here. e. I hate her so much sometimes I think I could kill her. Item 46. What might the therapist be saying to himself at this point? a. He really hasn't resolved his Oepidal hangups. b. He really hates her. She still is very important to him. c. He's trying very had to convince me. What's he trying to hide? d. I'd like to hear his mother's view. I bet it's a lot different. e. I really feel sorry for him. Still struggling with his mother at his age. Scene W THERAPIST - CLIENT Opening Statement (Therapist): "You seem to feel Time: 40 Seconds DO NOT TURN PAGE UNTIL THE SCENE IS OVER 206 Scene W THERAPIST - CLIENT (con't.) Closing Statement (Therapist): "What are you thinking?" Item 47. What do you think the therapist is saying to him- self at this point? a. I would really like to know what he is saying to himself at this point. b. I'm confused. If he's not feeling sad, what is he feeling? c. He's really not in touch with himself. I can't get him to admit his feelings. I'd better shift gears. d. He really is sad. So much he can't admit. He's getting overwhelmed by it. I'd better help him out. e. He's depressed. Is he thinking about suicide? I'd better check it out. Item 48. What might the client be saying to himself at this point? a. He sees right through me. I don't want to talk about it. b. I'm confused what does he want me to say? c. Sad? I've never been sad in my life. Where did he get that? d. If he only knew how sad I was, he wouldn't ask. I could cry, but I can't admit it. e. It's my mother's fault. If only she hadn't driven my friends away. Scene X THERAPIST - CLIENT Opening Statement (Therapist): "There was a time when she probably had a major respon- sibility." Time: 55 Seconds DO NOT TURN PAGE UNTIL THE SCENE IS OVER 207’ scene X THERAPIST - CLIENT (con't.) Closing Statement (Client): "Brother, I can't get to that answer yet, but I'm working on it." Item.49. What do you think the therapist is saying to himr self at this point? a. "Brother." That feels good. He's with me. we're working together. b. He is hard to work with. He takes away any- thing I give him. c. He is working on it, too hard maybe. d. That sure was a bunch of garbage he just shov- eled at me. He's got a thought disorder. e. How are you working on it? It doesn't sound like it to me! Item 50. What do you think the client is saying to himself at this point? a. I wish I knew. You're right on the money. b. What right does he have to tell me I'm locked into my mother? c. Locked into her? If he only knew I wanted to be even closer than I am! d. I'm working on it, so don't you push me. I'll get there. e. He's listening. I've got to be careful about what I say. Scene Y THERAPIST - CLIENT Opening Statement (Client): "People are supposed to have nice relationships with their mothers." Time: 30 Seconds DO NOT TURN PAGE UNTIL THE SCENE IS OVER Scene Y Item 51. Item 52. 208 THERAPIST - CLIENT Closing Statement (Client): "Maybe I misunderstood something." What do you think the client is saying to himself at this point? Get off it, buster. Everybody knows that. He's trying to make a fool out of me. He's getting too close. Is it possible? You mean I don't have to get along with her? What makes you think you know more about mothers than other people do? Everybody else believes that. ' What do you think the therapist is saying to himself at this point? a. b. Boy, is he denying. How am I ever going to get him in touch with his feelings. He has never really challenged that belief. He looks surprised that it can be challenged. He's not really believing it. He's putting me on, pretending he's willing to think about it. Maybe there's hope for him after all. He looks like he's willing to work on that. It's sad that it's taken so long for him to chal- lenge that message. It may be too late to make much of a difference. Scene Z Time: 55 Seconds THERAPIST - WOMAN CLIENT Opening Statement (Therapist): "Something this time enabled you to do, something that's hard to do." DO NOT TURN PAGE UNTIL THE SCENE IS OVER 209 Scene 2 THERAPIST - WOMAN CLIENT (con't.) Closing Statement (Therapist): "I don't like what I have had to see to become stronger." Item 53. What do you think the client is saying to herself at this point? a. And I resent having to work pp hard to get there. b. I guess that's why it's so hard to grow up - you have to be willing to look at the bad as well as the good. c. It's an ugly, dirty mess. I find people disgust- ing now. d. I've had to see that I'm alone. I've got to take care of me. It's not easy. e. I feel sad. This has really hurt. Item 54. What do you think the therapist is saying to himo self at this point? a. And I bet you resent me more than a little bit for forcing you to see it. b. What is it she's had to see? Reality? c. It's hard for her to admit that she's been avoid- ing seeing her parents as people. It's still hard. d. She's really sad as she says that. She's strug- gling to give them up. e. Her denial is still really strong. She even closed her eyes as she said that. Scene AA THERAPIST - WOMAN CLIENT Opening Statement (Client): "She went to school and she started crying." Time: 1 minute 20 Seconds DO NOT TURN PAGE UNTIL SCENE IS OVER 210 Scene AA THERAPIST - WOMAN CLIENT (con't.) Closing Statement (Client): "They're really nice." Item 55. What do you think the therapist is saying to himself at this point? a. That S.O.B. ex-husband. I'm pissed. I can imagine what she's feeling. b. It's humiliating. She's really hurting. c. Maybe now she'll be able to give up waiting for him to come around. d. I want her to know I understand. But I'm feel- ing I can't respond to her pain. e. Same old story. Somebody else is always respon- sible for her problems. It's exasperating to try to get her to see how she contributes. Item 56. What do you think the client is saying to herself at this point? a. He knows what I'm feeling. I don't feel so alone. b. I feel so helpless. So alone. c. I'm so ashamed. He really understands . . . but it hurts. d. I wish I could crawl away and hide right now. I'm so humiliated. e. I should have expected it. That's the way he is. No use staying upset. It's over and done with. Scene BB THERAPIST - WOMAN CLIENT Opening Statement (Client): "I don't know why I keep thinking that sometime . . ." Time: 20 Seconds DO NOT TURN PAGE UNTIL SCENE IS OVER 211 Scene BB THERAPIST - WOMAN CLIENT (con't.) Closing Statement (Client): "I guess I've hoped all my life." Item 57. What do you think the therapist is saying to him- self at this point? a. Hoped all her life? For what? b. It's hard for her to let go. She really gets scared. c. It's really hard for you to take responsibility for that. d. She's really a very helpless person. No wonder she can't give up hope. e. Prince Charming will never come, Cinderella. Stop waiting. Item 58. What do you think the client is saying to herself at this point? a. Is it hopeless? Must I give up? b. What is there if you don't have hope? What's left? c. Hoped in the wrong people, that's for sure. d. There's no future in continuing to hope. I must learn to live in the now. e. I wonder if he understands what I'm feeling. It scares me. Scene CC FAMILY Setting: Therapist - Older son - Younger son - Mother Opening Statement (Son): "Well, I was mad at you." Time: 1 minute 40 Seconds DO NOT TURN PAGE UNTIL SCENE IS OVER Scene CC Item 59. Item.60. Item 61. 212 FAMILY (con't.) Closing Statement (Mother): "Do you see the same thing?" What do you think the older son is saying to himself at this point? a. b. How can she say that? I haven't been happy since he left. I don't care what you just said. I'm still mad at you for getting the divorce. It's not fair. You (Mom) may be happier, but I'm sure not. It hurts so much to even know what Mbm just said. I don't agree. Nobody but her is getting any- thing out of it. What do you think the mother is saying to herself at this point? e. I wish I believed what I just said. He doesn't agree. How can I convince him that I did it for them, too? I wanted them to see it the way I do, but they don't. We've been over this so many times. When will the pain be gone? He must be angry with me. I've hurt him. What do you think the therapist is saying to himself at this point? a. b. She really wasn't very convincing - even to me. There's still a lot of pain here - all of them are still hurting about the divorce. Of course they don't see the same thing! How insensitive! Another divorce just so one person can feel better at the expense of the rest. .213 Scene CC FAMILY (con't.) Closing Statement (Mother): "Do you see the same thing?" Item 61. (con't.) e. They're having a hard time even listening to her. They must really be angry at her. Scene DD FAMILY (con't.) Setting: Therapist - Older Son - Younger Son - Mother Opening Statement (Therapist): "Want to talk about this . . ." (moves chair) 'Time: 50 Seconds DO NOT TURN PAGE UNTIL SCENE IS OVER Scene DD Item 62. Item 63. 214 FAMILY (con't.) Closing Statement (Older Son): "Grandpa and Grandma always fight, too." What do you think the mother is saying to herself at this point? a. I really feel guilty. It's made me happier but look what it's done to them. They really are angry at me. I can accept that. They're really hurt. I wonder if they'll ever understand. They don't believe me. They don't accept my statement. I'm feeling really helpless. Right now I don't know why I did get the divorce. I wish I hadn't. We could have made it somehow. What do you think the therapist is saying to himself at this point? a. Oh, oh. They're afraid that she might leave them, too, if she doesn't like what they are doing. They're more scared than angry. We gotta get this out. Mom, better pay atten- tion! They are really angry at her. It's important for her to hear that. I bet Grandma and Grandpa told him that. She must be catching it from all sides. Good. They're grieving. It's painful, but it's necessary. APPENDIX E NORMS FOR AFFECTIVE SENSITIVITY SCALE, FORM D Sunmary of Means and Standard Deviations - A.S.S. (Form D) - By Population .— O .— <5: I: o‘a'e >-u.l (I): o..- O (.0 z :3 O U INFORMAL EDUC- HEALTH ATION CARE CHILD MALE FEMALE GROUP DYAD ADULT INTER- VIEWER CLIENT IDENTIFICATION O00 ON 18.8 53.8 10.8 TOTALS 00'! ON a“) S.D. 1128 X MALES NO NM GO 2 5.0. 1862 X FEMALES mm ”N 12 HIGH EMPATHIZERS PSYCHOTHERAPISTS 215 40'- NM N"? OM ON UN 13. Y PSYCH. RESIDENTS 24 CO 000) NN on NO GM 71 FAMILY PRACTICE RESIDENTS ”’0 UN 4 3 3O 6 S.D. MEDICAL STUDENTS 330 ‘X NM NM 9 HOSPITAL STAFF Sun-nary of Means and Standard Deviations - A.S.S. - (Form D) - By Population (Continued) PSYCHO- TOTAL COUNS. INFORMAL EDUC- HEALTH DYAD INTER- ADULT CHILD MALE FEMALE GROUP CLIENT N IDENTIFICATION THERAPY CARE ATION VIEWER 57. 5.0 12. 17.5 10.2 2.9 5.5 2.7 UK can 46.5 15.9 10.6 4.1 11.2 3.9 24.5 HEALTH CARL PROF. POO NN TRAINLLS PARA-PROF. HEALTH CARE QC 01', RN can 52 TRAINEES PARAMLDICAL NO on 151 AIDtS MENTAL HEALTH 12115 NCO KN PK 0 0 out: N ‘2 but: in .— owe-- HH a ha.“ :6 01:2 8% F0 0N Mr- Q0 COUNSELORS SLX LDUC. Ch ON MN MM '— Olin TRAINING CHAPLAIN IN '0! MN SCHOOL COUNSELORS 64 0° 000', 00 our 137 TRAINEE SCHOOL COUNS. RN WM 5.0. I? 39 STUOtNTS (M.S.) GRAD COINS. I‘M $0M mo 05¢ 21 GRAD COUNS. STUUtNTS (PH.D) Summary of Means and Standard Deviations - A.S.S. (Form D) - By Population (Continued) TOTAL PSYCHO- THERAPY INFORMAL COUNS. HEALTH EDUC- ATION FEMALE GROUP DYAD MALE CHILD CLIENT INTER- ADULT IDENTIFICATION CARE VIEHER FF QN SUPERVISORS EDUCATIONAL o—O NM 44 ‘Y W m u 2 H z ”5 0-0- a Ema aqub: “5.» 9(2) zg< H V X mo 0M 73 TEACHER TRAINEES 217 G'O NM 3 S.D. 592 X- COLLEGE UNDER- GRADUATES MO I‘M 3 5.0 170 'Y SOCIAL HORK UNDERGRADUATES NR RN Rh P P 238 '7 HSU STUDENTS IN IPR O!— ‘0" 60 COMMUNITY (CANADA) CORRECTION Summary of Means and Standard Deviations - A.S.S. (Form D) - Medical Total thera Dyad Education Health Informal Counaeling Payche- Care Female Group Mala Inter- Adult Child viewer Client Identification Medical Students Group 9 j 6.6 2.7 11.6 3.5 18.7 4.3 55.4 0.2 14.8 3.6 67 i 30.8 S.D. 5a Medical Students 1976 - Mich. 42 s] 11.2 13.3 52.7 19.7 9.1 6.1 28.5 27.9 29.8 :0? Clin. Medical Studenta 55 2.7 3.6 4.6 6.3 12.9 2.5 8.6 5.4 4.3 10.6 4.7 5.4 S.D. 7.2 2.6 12.1 3.6 9 3 752 30. s.0. 6. Medical Studenta 1975 - Mich. 64 0 5 Medical Studenta 44 i 8 S.D. 4 Kanaaa 66 218 Medical Studenta 35 i Ohio 70 30.3 27.8 57.2 10.7 38.8 29.1 14.1 53.8 72 X Medical Students NOD. 72 5.3 10.2 2.9 7.8 5.7 3.8 9.9 6.3 S.D. 29.2 6.2 ' 30.0 5.0. 6.1 Community Medical 47 X Studenta 1977 Mich. 86 ’ 28.9 28.0 56.5 34 X Medical Studenta 89 6.2 12.1 6.8 S.D. Ohio Sumnary of Means and Standard Deviations - A.S.S.: (Form D) - Medical Residents Total therapy Counaeling Paycho- Informal Care Dyad Education Health Female Group Male Child Inter- Adult viewer Client N P Practice Reaidenta Group Identification # I00 on 6.5 3.2 9.2 3.8 17.3 3. .5 10.4 3.5 13.0 25.9 5.6 .6 8.2 9.2 2.7 52.3 11.2 5.2 26.3 6.2 S.D. Paldly Practice 47 x Reaidanta 2 59.7 17.0 0—8 mm N0 32.5 11.7 8.8 1.9 50.9 15.7 24.7 7.3 26.5 . 8.5 S.D Palily Practice 11 X Reaidanta, 8.0. 25 N0 00'! Family Practice Raaidanta, lanaaa 43 on @M In, ON 13 leaidanta, lanaaa '35 Palily Practice 219 o v. in. u u :8 1:1! U'd - a: 8. 3. 0“” InN Raaidenta-Mich. Paychiatry 3 «an 90-1 on {N (HI-0 NM leaidanta , Kanaaa Paychiatry 26 NO IfiN u i' Paychiatry Raaidenta, Minn. 37 Summary of Means and Standard Deviations - A.S.S. (Form D) - Mental Health Paraprofessional Dyed Education Health Total there Informal Counaelin; Payche- Care Gr0up Female Male Inter- Adult Child Viewer Client N Health Care jCon't) Identification Group 9 040 ON WM 46 i 5.0. Mental Health Therapy Aida, M.Y. 14 F‘N ON IflI-l on 15.6 3.9 45.3 8.4 13.5 7.0 26.1 4.6 32.6 7.2 7.9 3.5 50.9 9.3 5.3 24.3 25.3 4.4 S.D. Mental Health Therapy Aida, N.Y. 29 MM OM 00' '00"! 11.1 3.2 51.4 13.0 25.1 6.2 28.0 6.6 S.D. 1% Mental Health Therapy Aida, I.Y. 31 00-. RN 30.2 3.6 43.6 6.5 2.9 11.2 62.6 6.8 32.0 4.4 4.3 31.3 1053 Therapy Aida, M.Y. Mental Health 32 NM ‘06" Oh N as i 8.1). Therapy Aida, M.Y. Mental Health 61 MO RN NM OI"! 890-0 00 N S.D. 18 X Mental Health Therapy Aida, M.Y. 74 MM 0M 28.5 6.1 33.0 S.D. 7.7 17 Therapy Aide, N.Y. Mental Health 91 221 evauoum o eenuauu o.n~ o.o n.« o.~ o.e «.8 «.o~ N.e n.o n.o o.~ o.- 0.0 ”.5 .n.m uncouaeeuounauem o.oo n.a~ o.o~ ¢.s o.o~ ~.w~ ~.nn n.n~ c.n~ ~.on o.o~ a.nn s.ou n.o~ m.on pug-e: deuce: ooH luauofl— I Gouda; e.- c.c O.“ s.u ~.n o.n n.o~ c.n «.n o.“ <.n o.o ¢.n 5.0 .a.m uaaoaaaeuoumeuem n.0o n.0u n.o~ o.o n.- ~.o~ c.nm ~.n~ «.mu ¢.¢n ”.0“ n.on a.- 0.0“ .m on nu~eeu deuce: oo Godheab I coca-ha. o.- n.n «.n n.n ¢.n d.c n.o «.e o.n n.n <.« n.ou «.0 o.n .a.m anneaeeeuouneuem n.no o.o~ n.cu A.“ 8.0” o.w~ o.~n a.n~ n.c~ n.5n o.o~ m.nm o.n~ n.»« .m on apnea: deuce: no eouuogh u eemueua o.- s.¢ a." n.n m.< n.n n.a o.n n.¢ a.» u.~ o.o n.n s.n .n.m mecca-eeuouaeuem «.2. ~.N~ n2: mg n..: 0.2 5K... 5.: «.3 oéo n.o~ 0.3 0.0..” «.2 m «N named: any-.0! on naeuaAu eueo u930«> .u.cou .eueo guano: * ~euoh somehow nuaueanoou Haauouou Juana: nodueusvm wean noouo nausea can: vgasu uH=v< auoucu unowuo. z mogueuuuuumevou macho Aumacwucouv mpmcovmmmwotamcmm smem: Pause: - Ac stony .m.m.< - mcompmw>ma vcmvcmum can meow: we >um523m Summary of Means and Standard Deviations - A.S.S. (Form D) - Occupational Therapy Trainees Total Health Internal Counseling Psycho- Pemale Group Dyad Education Male Inter- Adult Child Client Indentification Group there Care 4__ viewer Health Care. Con't. i “‘0 RN 29.2 0 O 5.6 103 i Occupational Therapist -Aust. 20 66.0 12.0 u-afl EN 00 In 99E Therapist -Aust. Occupational 49 00 ON 575? Therapist oAuat. Occupational 90 222 “D F. O" ”Q "N QM OM 9.1 37.0 2.6 55.1 10.3 27.2 5. 27.5 - 6.2 53 SE 3.!) Occupational Therapist -Aust. 101 Ina GM 3 . 49 X S.D Occupatinal Therapist -Auat. 102 ‘00 MN 9.2 2.3 55.3 11.2 27.5 28.3 . 5.3 58 i 8.0 Therapist -Aust. OccupationII 103 Summary of Means and Standard Deviations - A.S.S. (Form D) - Health Professional and Paraprofessionals Total Informal Counseling Psycho- Dyad Education Health Group Female Male Inter- Adult Child Client N Group Identification there Care Viewer Health Care # MO ON 00 IflM 6.1 2.7 13.8 3.7 8.0 41.2 9.6 3.6 20.4 4.9 30.5 8.5 NB ”N 13 i 22.4 S.D. 4.2 Paramedical Trainees, Aust. 47 66.2 11.3 19.4 5.3 10.5 2.7 6.6 2.8 10.9 3.4 18.7 3.9 52.1 8.8 14.1 3.8 28.3 5.7 37.8 7.9 10.3 2.6 55.8 10.2 29.7 27.0 S.D. 5.9 6.1 392 Phyicians, Aids, N.C. 69 10.1 7.3 10.4 29.6 59.0 11.9 39.8 31.1 14.9 56.0 21.6 31.3 92' Hospital Staff, Mich. 13 3.3 2.1 6.4 6.5 3.1 7.5 3.9 2.0 8.5 4.0 3.1 5.4 S.D. H0 MN Q’s-l NM 9.9 2.8 46.4 10.1 22.6 6.5 5.5 26.7 Health Professionals x 18 S.D. Aunt. 11 223 9.5 3.1 5.4 2.8 8.9 3.6 15.5 3.9 46.1 3.6 11.4 10.5 23.9 5.3 32.6 10.1 8.6 2.5 47.9 12.3 23.2 7.1 25. 6.4 Health Professionals X 38 S.D. Aust. 38 6.2 10.2 15.8 56.6 9.2 4.2 45.0 15.1 25.0 11.6 48.0 8.5 31.6 2.4 23.1 24.4 Health Professionals i 87 4.7 13.5 2.9 2.8 3.9 11.2- 7.1 3.7 7.7 12.2 5.9 6.7 27 S.D. Aust. 71. 11. 7.1 11.2 22.4 2.9 11.1 14.1 57.6 19.9 31.8 30.1 60.8 10.9 1.4 30.3 7.5 asi Mental Health 23 4.1 2.3 3.7 8.9 4.2 3.8 5.5 5.0 5.8 9.9 2.6 8.0 Paraprof Trainee Florida 65.0 7.1 10.2 18.8 2.7 11.5 28.2 13.9 51.1 36.8 9.7 2.7 9.3 26.3 28.9 37? Metnal Health 41 4.9 10.4 2.5 3.5 5.5 3.9 8.2 4.5 7.2 5.7 . 4.7 S.D Paraprof Trainee Florida Health Professionals Auat. 6 {h MN \00 OM Summary of Means and Standard Deviations - A.S.S. (Form D) - Counselors Total Counseling Psycho- Dyad Education Health Informal Inter- Adult Child Male Female Group Client Identification Counselors Group there Care viewer # 12.7 48.4 16.6 8.3 6.1 10.4 19.7 61.1 4.1 26.3 9.3 34.8 3.2 11.1 51.8 24.6 Sex Attitude 21 28.0 36 5.9 15.3 2.9 2.9 3.7 13.4 4.9 6.3 7.7 7.3 13.6 8.0 Counselors - N.D. 59.4 15.3 ~30 ON MN QM Q0 ”N Ix 9 0m Paraprofessional Counselors 40 14.4 9.4 7.3 9.4 19.3 60.0 3.2 50.1 25.3 9.9 8.3 34.7 24.7 51.7 2.8 .8 25 Chaplains-in training 45 12.5 4.1 2.7 4.4 10.5 5.8 3.4 8.6 6.4 4.9 10.6 S.D. OM 11.5 3.0 7.4 2.9 11.2 3.9 19.4 3.9 14.0 56.3 6.2 3.8 11.9 29.8 9.9 10.8 40.5 3.0 59.5 12.5 29.0 6.2 30.9 7.6 School Counselors 64 i NOY‘ 4 0” ON 54.2 12.1 26.4 6.1 27.1 7.0 85 X 5.0. School Counselor Trainees - Auat. 7 224 Nu-I O .4 22.3 5.1 9.5 3.0 5.6 2.9 9.7 3.6 17.1 3.4 25.2 13.2 51.0 2.3 7.8 4.0 2.4 9.7 10.2 39.0 54.0 8.8 27.3 5.9 28.5 4.3 10 X S.D. School Counselor Trainees - Aust. 60.5 13.8 16.2 8.6 6.8 11.0 17.9 3.8 49.4 26.5 8.8 34.0 26.6 24.4 51.7 2.8 42E School Counselor Trainees 3.1 5.0 3.3 4.7 3.3 11.8 6.4 6.8 11.9 10.0 5.5 8.0 ' Auste 69.6 12.6 20.3 5.5 7.3 2.6 11.8 4.3 18.0 4.7 55.9 10.0 13.7 3.8 30.4 5.2 39.3 8.7 9.7 2.7 59.9 7.0 10.9 28.6 31.0 5.6 28 X S.D. Grad Counseling Students, MS 52 10.6 18.9 60.7 6.0 4.0 11.6 49.1 16.1 9.1 26.2 8.3 34.5 25.5 26.6 52.4 1635 Grad. Counseling Students - M.S. 5.4 13.3 3.1 5.7 3.4 11.5 3.4 3.4 2.7 8.4 12.0 6.2 4.7 8.0 12.2 22.0 73.8 7.3 2.3 30.1 14.7 59.2 20.0 12.4 11.2 43.7 62.6 11.9 32.2 31.5 23)? Grad. Counseling Students - M.A. 4.5 14.1 3.1 6.0 3.9 11.8 4.4 4.6 9.0 3.4 8.0 6.4 S.D. 27.4 77.6 13.3 9.5 6.1 4.5 21.4 60.0 35.4 85? Grad. Counseling Students - Ph.D. 3.7 11.1 2.0 3.6 3.7 9.7 5.3 4.5 4.8 10.9 1.7 8.0 10.4 444.3 10.7 15.0 57.2 18.4 9.5 7.1 27.9 30.5 31.2 61.8 13 X Grad. Counseling Students - Ph.D. 96 2.9 3.8 3.2 3.1 10.6 3.4 5.8 7.6 2.6 10.9 7.4 8.0 A.S.S. (Form D) - Educators Summary of Means and Standard Deviations Total therapy Counseling Psycho- Health Informal Care Dyad Education Male Female Group Adult Child Client Inter- Viewer Identification Educators Group # 69.9 28.4 14.3 55.6 18.2 11.6 6.9 11.1 22.1 29.6 30.3 60.6 9.3 41.5 3.7 Graduate Counselor 14 x Education Students 46 12.8 5.6 2.0 5.5 3.5 2.6 11.2 5.4 3.5 8.7 7.9 5.1 10.2 0 OM ON 31.9 4.9 43.7 7.2 11.9 2.7 63.8 9.5 32.1 5.7 32.6 5.5 Graduate Educational 25 X Psychology Students 3.0 84 72.8 12.9 19.9 31.4 13.5 59.3 19.7 13.5 6.9 2.9 30e1 62.5 10.3 41.4 33.0 Kindergarten teacher 15 X trainees - Aust. 63 10.0 4.8 2.6 2.6 8.8 3.1 6.4 5.2 3.2 7.6 4.1 4.9 4.9 S.D. 70. 12. 21.2 30.2 14.8 55.3 19.8 11.3 6.3 10.9 39.9 59.2 31.2 Teacher trainees 53 i Mich. 94 4.9 2.3 5.9 3.8 10.4 4.5 3.5 3.1 9.0 2.6 11.8 5.9 5.7 73.9 31.4 63.0 11.0 41.7 32.2 15.3 58.7 21.2 12.0 7.3 11.7 21.8 32.3 20?? Parents 97 10.3 2.9 4.2 3.0 2.8 8.9 3.8 3.3 4.6 5.7 8.8 2.0 5.8 5.6 S.D. Educational 8.5 9.4 57.6 11.5 40.5 28.5 13.5 55.5 20.2 28.2 .6 30 Supervisors - Ill.13 X 6.6 6.2 12.0 2.1 8.5 6.3 4.2 9.9 3.4 3.2 S.D. Educational 16 7.8 2.1 59.1 10.1 40.7 28.4 12.1 57.1 17.9 10.1 28.2 .7 30 Supervisors - 111.20 X 3.8 8.6 4.1 4.3 6.3 5.6 9.3 2.9 7.0 5.3 S.D. Ns-l I‘M 8 5 29. 5. Kindergarten teacher 29 x. trainees - Aust. S.D. 92 Summary of Means and Standard Deviations - A.S.S. (Form D) - Students 226 s-I v-ON MM [‘0 MN OM e e e e e a a a e e 3 ON NN MN MO OM ‘9. 90—0 [\c-I 90-4 90—0 90—0 0 O§ OF! RN Oo-l 00 e e e a a e e a s e .3. OM OM OM HM OM g.” N N Fl 0 .1: a.“ v0 a-I 00‘ Oh O'O MM GM 0 e a a e a e e e a a o-ON v-‘N o-ON u-ON on a and H s-d O U H E 06 OM 0N I-‘Q OF! I O O I I O O O O I 8 [NM MN OM INN NM 6 H £0 u-il‘ OM MM OM FOO H“ e a a e a e e e a e I6 '4” NM OM I-IM OM “O F‘ H all! .4 :1: fl .3 MO QN NQ GO 0 U 0 I e a e e e e a a Q OM OQ c-aflt Os? 0“? 0 e-I N H :3 ‘U N '3 he OM OM ON ‘00 O I O O O O O O O O as §O O0 MO #0 ¢—s MH M Mo-O Mr. O- x? GO NM £5 e-IQ a e e e e as as ea 0 QM \fs‘t NM QM 42¢ a... H o-‘ H H PI 0 0 .3 . «so: aha. uacu \o<3 unun O O I O O I O I O C 5 GM F‘M 0O OM 00 N M N ha 0 ”Q #M OM NM NG p-l e e e e e e a e a e i 00 v-‘M GO ON 00 M Q M M M 'U {G ON ON OM OM d e e a I e e e e e a H ON ON ON ON 09M .6 P‘ H F. U 0 NO as!" OM MM O H a e e e e e a e e e :3 wo-o HO Inv-I an an '0 MP. 991-9 MP! M MP! < I u was no so MI-i «<7 ‘5 e e e e e e a e a a 0g OM OVD [‘0 GM 00 U N M N N Dd H> U OM NN OF: 00 NC: a 00 H0 GO GM 09" H M M N N N e-l O O Q O 9 IX ' IN ° |>< ' IN 9 Ix ° \Dm Nm Q Om Om z \D N M M M .4 N ed e e .3 .fl 0 O n 3 3 a dig :32 3 ‘1' SJ U v0 v. a: Q g I I I Q =3 :1 Q I: 0 '0 '0 'U '6. '0. *0. U I I .0 Sn mu h h a a. MC Vic w w e e-l find 80' H in s In“ no 5‘0 5'0 0 9 0.6 0v! 0 0 OD 'O>‘ 'UU 'Uo-t ‘0“ '08 'DU 8 ' {:H ‘2. £91 an. HO: 2 :33: :30 H DH :- Q In pt (I) 0% H M m to (D 71.3 12.1 70.7 12.6 20.8 4.9 21.3 4.9 9.8 3.1 11.3 2.5 11.6 2.9 7.0 2.9 7.4 2.7 7.5 3.0 7.1 3.0 10.9 4.2 11.8 3.6 11.1 3.8 11.2 3.5 18.8 4.8 4.0 20.6 4.1 19.5 4.5 52.3 12.8 56.6 9.2 56.5 10.1 56.4 11.1 13.5 4.1 14.7 3.8 14.8 5.6 14.3 3.2 28.5 7.0 30.4 5.6 30.3 6.2 30.1 6.3 37.3 10.2 40.9 7.8 41.0 7.8 40.6 7.7 10.2 3.1 10.2 2.9 11.0 2.6 10.6 2.9 55.7 14.0 62.8 12.1 61.0 9.9 60.2 10.7 60.1 10.8 27.0 7.1 31.0 6.0 30.5 5.2 30.6 5.4 29.8 5.7 29.1 7.5 31.5 8.1 30.4 6.1 30.1 6.3 30.8 6.6 72 x s.p. s.o. 78 i s.o. SID. 17? 937 47} IPR Class - Mich. IPR Class - Mich. IPR Class - Mich. Undergrads in Undergrads in Undergrads in Social Work Undergrads Social Work Undergrads 10 79 82 77 78 Summary of Means and Standard Deviations - A.S.S. (Form D) - Students (Continued) and Correction Workers Total Counseling Psycho- Health Informal Dyad Education Male Fremale Group Adult Child Client Inter- Identification N Group therggy Care Viewer L Students (can't) # 29.0 30.6 58.8 9.9 41.5 27.1 14.6 54.0 18.0 12.0 6.1 a? Social Work undergrads 95 8.7 9.5 18.2 3.4 12.9 7.9 4.1 16.7 6.9 5.2 4.0 S.D. 63. 14. 12.0 18.2 28.3 26.7 54.3 9.2 36.5 26.9 11.6 51.8 16.4 10.6 6.3 Students 12 i 8. Ohio 58 5.9 2.8 3.5 10.9 5.2 2.0 12.5 4.2 4.1 2.6 12.3 6.2 7.1 S.D. 227 Correction Community 12 Correction Community 58.6 17.7 9.6 26.1 10.7 47.9 17.2 9.1 5.0 5.1 32.4 27.2 23.4 49.0 9.6 9)? Canada 7.1 4.4 3.0 1.0 5.0 4.3 2.4 4.1 3.2 4.3 7.1 5.3 24 Correction Community Canada 64.3 12.1 19.8 6.6 3.1 28.6 26.1 54.4 9.8 37.3 26.9 12.6 51.7 17.2 8.6 7.2 167? 13.7 4.9 1.9 10.8 5.3 3.8 4.7 6.3 11.6 2.6 9.2 6.8 67.9 12.4 21.1 6.3 3.1 26.8 57.2 10.7 38.6 29.3 13.9 54.0 17.8 10.3 31. 15 X Correc tions Comuni ty_ Canada 14.9 5.6 3.2 3.1 2.9 10.3 6.0 4.1 12.4 5.1 12.9 7.2 7.5 81 Correc ti on Comuni ty Canada 63.9 10.8 18.6 51.5 18.2 10.6 5.8 12.4 27.6 27.1 53.6 10.3 .5 28 207 8.0 llol 4.8 2.6 3.5 4.9 3.4 9.8 3.4 4.2 3.0 10.1 4.2 5.4 APPENDIX F INTER-ITEM CORRELATIONS 228 Appendix F. Inter-Ital Correlations of Affectivn Sensitivity Scale rbrl D 1 1.00 2 -,o7 1.00 3 -.03 .02 1.00 4 .03 .00 -.09 1.00 5 -.03 .02 .06 .02 1.00 5 -.03 .09 .1o -.01 .11 1.00 7 .01 .01 .03 .01 -.02 .01 1.00 a -.02 .01 .07 -.oz .03 .01 .06 1.00 9 -.03 .07 .06 -.01 .01 .05 .05 .06 1.00 10 .01 .04 .08 .oo -.01 .01 .05 .03 .11 1.00 11 .02 -.04 .04 -.oo .02 .oo .03 -.oo .04 -.05 1.00 12 —.01 .07 .os .01 .01 .03 .06 .03 .06 .05 —-01 1.00 13 .oo .03 .13 -.oo .02 -.oo .03 .03 .oo .03 -.01 .07 1.00 14 .05 .05 .oo .oo .01 .os .07 .04 .07 .12 .02 .05 .06 1.00 15 .02 .04 .06 -.03 -.oo .02 .01 .08 .os .07 -o1 .02 .07 .28 1.00 16 -.01 .05 .02 .01 .01 .04 .06 .02 .04 -.oo ~03 .03 -.oo .01 .04 1.00 17 -.02 .02 .02 -.03 -.04 -03 -02 .os .03 .01 -01 .07 -.01 .08 .12 .08 1.00 13 -.02 .06 .09 -.04 .oo .08 .1o .08 .11 .06 --02 .13 .05 .17 .16 .07 .12 1.00 19 -.oz .04 .05 —.01 -.02 .oo .09 .11 .07 .05 -01 .07 .06 .14 .12 .08 .12 .32 1.00 20 -.oo .05 .04 .01 .03 .04 .04 .05 .06 .06 -.oo .09 .04 .08 .06 .02 .06 .15 .09 1.00 21 .02 .04 .06 .02 -.oo .04 .04 .05 .12 .06 -.01 .03 .06 .07 .07 .08 .06 .16 .11 .19 1.00 22 -.00 ~04- .06 --01 .04 .01 '-°1 .02 .01 .04 .03 -.02 .oo .07 .04 .02 .03 .02 .05 .02 .07 23 -.02 .03 .08 -.06 -.01 .02 .03 .06 .07 .04 .02 .09 .07 .12 .11 .03 .07 .15 .16 .09 .09 24 -.00 -°1 .06 .00 .05 -07 -02 .01 .09 .05 .02 .02 -.01 .12 .09 .04 .05 .1o .07 .03 .12 25 -.03 .01 .04 .04 .04 .02 .03 .02 .07 .02 .04 .06 -.01 .07 .03 .04 .04 .12 .11 .06 .09 26 .01 --01 -.01 .00 .01 .03 -.00 -.oz -.04 .oo .04 .00 -.oz .00 -.02 -.03 -.os -.05 -.04 -.02 -.01 27 -.02 .06 .02 .03 .01 .09 .os .05 .06 .02 .03 .1o .07 .06 .07 .06 .02 .11 .08 .06 .12 23 -.06 .06 .08 -.01 .oo .07 .06 .04 .08 .03 -.01 .12 .06 .08 .os .07 .06 .17 .12 .07 .11 29 -.01 .06 .1o -.03 .03 .11 .07 .05 .09 .09 .01 .1o .02 .14 .09 .06 .06 .18 .16 .06 .10 3o -.02 .05 .05 -.02 .01 .03 .06 .01 .07 .06 .03 .06 .01 .09 .07 .os .04 .1o .09 .03 .04 31 .oo .04 .07 .01 .01 .03 .02 -.oo .06 .04 .01 .04 .02 .04 .02 .04 .03 .09 .02 .07 .09 32 -.03 .01 .04 -.01 .02 .07 .02 -.oo .05 .03 .02 .01 -.02 .07 .04 .02 .oo .05 .oo .04 .01 33 .02 .04 .03 -.oo .04 .03 .07 .07 .07 .04 -.oz .07 .os .05 .04 .06 .08 .12 '.o9 .06 .09 34 —.03 .07 .04 -.02 -.01 .06 .13 .10 .07 .04 -.01 .13 .06 .1o .09 .06 .09 .16 .11 .08 .1o 35 -.02 -.01 .01 -.01 -.oo -.02 -.02 .04 .03 .02 .01 .01 -.03 -.oz -.03 -.01 -.oo .01 .oo .03 .01 36 .00 .04 .06 .00 .oo .09 .05 .oo .05 .04 .01 .04 .01 .06 .03 .05 .03 .05 .06 .06 .08 37 -.03 .08 .06 -.03 .03 .06 .04 .04 .07 .06 .05 .07 .02 .06 .07 .os .07 .10 .12 .os .07 38 -.03 .06 .05 -.02 .02 .08 .oo .os .03 .06 .01 .06 .03 .12 .06 .04 .06 .12 .12 .07 .15 39 .04 .00 .05 .02 .02 .03 -.03 .04 .03 .03 .02 .02 -.o3 .06 .05 -.oo .01 .05 .01 .04 .06 4o .00 .02 .03 .oo .oo .01 -.01 -.oo .03 .06 -.02 .04 .05 .04 .06 -.03 .03 .06 -.01 .05 .02 41 .03 -.01 -.01 .04 .02 .01 -.02 .03 -.01 -.03 -.02 .01 -.oz -.02 .oo -.01 -.oz -.oz .oo .01 -.03 42 -.03 .04 .03 -.01 .02 .05 .06 .04 .08 .07 .oo .03 .07 .13 .06 .03 .04 .12 .1o .06 .03 43 -.01 .05 .08 -.03 -.oo .07 .09 .05 .05 .02 .03 .07 .05 .10 .os .07 .03 .13 .09 .03 .08 44 .03 .06 .06 .02 .01 .11 .07 .03 .08 .02 .02 .04 .01 .06 .06 .os .01 .07 .06 .06 .1o 45 -.o4 .02 .02 .01 .02 .01 .03 .os .04 .06 -.02 .06 .06 .08 .02 -.o1 .02 .09 .04 .06 .09 46 -.01 .00 .08 -.00 .03 .02 .01 .01 .01 .03 .06 -.02 -.05 .06 .06 .05 .02 .03 .03 .02 .02 47 -.oo -.01 .09 -.oo -.oo -.01 .03 .01 .04 .03 -.oz .05 .02 .07 .05 .oo .07 .08 .09 .02 .06 48 -.00 '-°° .00 .00 .01 -01 .02 -.00 -.02 -.02 .04 .oo -.03 .03 .oo .02 .01 -.03 -.04 -.06 .01 49 -.03 .04 .06 -.01 .04 .07 .06 .03 .09 .03 .02 .06 .04 .13 .11 .04 .05 .15 .11 .05 .13 so -.00 .03 .06 -.01 -.01 .04 .03 .04 .06 .05 .oo .04 .05 .12 .14 .01 .09 .15 .13 .07 .08 51 -.02 .02 .04 -.o1 .01 .01. .06 .os .07 .06 -.01 .10 .os .07 .04 .02 .04 .13 .13 .06 .05 52 -.01 .07 .06 .01 .03 .04 .03 .os .08 .07 -.04 .06 .01 .07 .08 .03 .02 .09 .06 .06 .08 53 -.02 .06 .10 .oo .03 .03 .05 .03 .04 .06 .03 .09 .04 .1o .02 .03 .07 .11 .09 .08 .08 54 —.03 -.01 .06 .03 .03 .02 -.oo .02 .06 .os .01 .03 .01 .06 .05 -.02 .02 .06 .09 .04 .oo ss -.01 -.01 .07 -.oz -.01 .01 .02 .01 .os .05 .oo .02 .01 .06 .04 .os .05 .06 .06 .09 .06 56 .03 -.01 .04 -.oo -.02 .04 .04 .02 .05 .06 .01 .02 .01 .03 .04 .03 .02 .07 .04 .01 .05 57 -.05 .01 .09 .02 .os .os .os -.01 .1o .05 .04 .07 .02 .08 .04 .01 -.oo .09 .07 .04 .05 58 .oo .00 .03 .01 .01 -.01 .01 .03 .04 .oz -.o1 .02 .03 .06 .03 -.o1 .01 .07 .02 .os .04 59 -.01 .04 .01 -.01 ‘.01 .02 .03 .01 .06 .02 .oo .03 .03 .02 .03 .02 .02 .08 .06 .oo .07 60 .04 .03 -.o1 .02 .02 .01 .04 .oo .01 .07 .01 .01 .05 .04 .04 -.01 -.03 .03 -.01 .01 .02 61 .02 .07 .06 .01 .05 .04 .07 .06 .09 .06 .01 .07 .04 .09 .06 .06 .03 .1o .11 .04 .1o 62 .01 .08 .os -.00 .02 .03 .05 .02 .09 .09 -.03 .08 .04 .19 .03 .01 .04 .11 .09 .03 .12 63 -.00 .03 .01 .02 .01 .01 -.oo .01 -.01 -.01 -.02 .01 .03 .02 .04 -.oo -.02 .03 .oo .01 .01 229 Appendix F. (Continued) 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 4O 41 42 22 1.00 23 .05 1.00 24 .10 .08 1.00 25 .04 .09 .18 1.00 26 .02 -.03 -.02 -.oo 1.00 27 .01 .05 .10 .10 .02 1,00 28 -.01 .11 .07 .10 ‘-03 .31 1.00 29 .15 .14 .11 .10 -.OS .13 .21 1.00 30 .04 .11 .08 .06 ”.03 .13 -,15 .25 1_oo 31 .00 .01 .06 .04 -.O4 .01 .06 .09 .07 1.00 32 .04 .01 .10 .04 -.02 .03 .04 .03 .07 .09 1,00 33 .00 .08 .06 .01 -.04 .08 .10 .13 .06 .05 -.01 1.00 34 .04 .14 -06 .09 '-°3 .08 .11 .16 .09 .05 .oo .23 1.00 35 .01 .02 .01 .02 .01 -.01 -.03 -,02 .02 -,02 .02 .03 .04 1.00 36 .05 -03 -11 -11 -°1 .08 .08 .09 .05 .03 .06 .09 .12 -03 1-00 37 .04 .09 .03 .06 .02 .09 .10 .11 .09 .03 .03 .05 .14 .02 .08 1.00 38 .02 .09 .07 .06 --01 .10 .11 .14 .09 .05 .04 .10 .13 -.01 .11 .25 1.00 39 .01 .07 .05 .02 -.03 .02 .00 .03 .os -.03 .04 .05 .05 .01 -.01 .02 .04 1.00 40 .05 .01 .02 .01 -.03 .00 -.02 .05 .02 .05 .03 -,03 .03 -.02 .02 .03 .03 .09 1.00 41 -.02 .00 .03 .01 .03 .01 .03 .02 -,01 .03 -,oo .02 -_oz .00 .03 .01 -.01 .01 -.02 1.00 42 .04 .07 .08 .12 -.01 .13 .13 .15 .08 .05 .05 .05 .03 -.oo .05 .10 .12 .04 .01 .06 1.00 43 .03 .08 .07 .05 .01 .10 .12 .14 .10 .06 .04 .06 .11 -.02 .03 .11 .10 .01 .00 -.02 .12 44 .01 .02 .08 .03 '-02 .07 .09 .09 .03 .07 .05 .08 .06 .01 .05 .10 .08 .03 .01 .00 .09 45 .00 .08 .07 .08 ‘-03 .02 .05 .04 .01 .04 .02 .05 .03 .01 .04 .04 .07 .01 .03 -.02 .06 46 .06 .00 .10 .05 .01 .04 -.01 .10 .05 -,02 .03 .01 -,oo .04 .03 .08 .06 .01 .01 .01 .01 47 .02 .06 .06 .08 --01 .06 .06 .03 .02 -,oo .01 .05 .05 .00 .05 .05 .05 .02 .01 .OO .06 48 .00 -.02 .00 .04 .00 .01 .01 .01 —.02 ,oo .03 -,01 .,03 -.01 .00 .02 -.oo -.06 -.O4 .02 .05 49 .02 .09 .05 .09 -.02 .11 .11 .13 .05 .06 .02 .09 .11 -.02 .04 .09 .09 .03 .01 -.01 .12 50 .09 .14 .07 .06 “.02 .04 .08 .11 .05 .01 _01 .05 .11 -.02 .06 .09 .07 .04 .02 .02 .08 51 .01 .09 .06 .05 -.OO .09 .11 .15 .03 .02 .02 .11 .12 -.O4 .05 .09 .09 .02 .01 .01 .12 52 .01 .06 .07 .08 -.05 .08 .OS .09 .05 .03 .03 .07 ;05 -.03 .03 .05 .07 .03 .02 .03 .09 S3 .02 .17 .09 .05 -.01 .09 .12 .13 ,04 .05 .05 .05 ,07 .03 .06 .05 .05 .05 .03 -.03 .11 54 .01 .05 .05 .07 .02 .05 .04 .05 .04 -.01 .05 .01 .03 .04 .04 .07 .05 .01 .Ol .03 .03 55 .04 .08 .05 .07 -.02 .00 .03 .04 .03 .04 .03 .02 .03 .00 .04 .04 .06 .03 .01 -.Ol .09 56 .02 .02 .06 .06 ‘-02 .06 .06 .04 .05 .03 -,03 .03 .03 -.Ol .02 .04 .05 .01 -.OS .02 .03 S7 .05 .06 .09 .06 -.03 .07 .07 .09 .03 .04 .03 .05 .02 .05 .06 .08 .07 .00 .06 -.OO .09 58 .02 .01 .01 .04 .01 .03 -.01 .04 .02 .05 -.02 .04 .01 .01 .01 .01 .04 .02 .04 .06 .04 59 .00 .01 .05 .05 -.02 .04 .06 .04 ,03‘ .03 -,02 ,07 .03 -.01 .03 .07 .04 -.01 .04 .03 .04 60 .06 .05 .02 .01 .00 .03 .01 -.oo .05 .01 .05 —,oo .03 -.oo .01 .04 .03 .02 .03 .02 .05 61 .03 .08 .09 .09 -.O4 .08 .09 .10 .05 .07 .04 .03 .09 .05 .09 .09 .09 .04 .02 .05 .10 62 .01 .08 .08 .07 -.02 .05 .08 .11 .05 .05 .03 .07 .07 .05 .06 .06 .06 .05 .07 -.02 .ll 63 .oo .02 .04 .03 -00 .04 .02 .01 .01 .01 .03 -,04 ,oo .03 f.01 .03 -.00 .04 .02 -.02 -.00 230 Appendix F. (Continued) 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 SO 51 52 53 S4 55 56 S7 58 59 6O 61 62 63 43 1.00 44 .23 1.00 45 .01 .02 1.00 46 .06 .04 -.02 1.00 47 .05 .00 .07 .06 1.00 48 .04 .66 .02 .02 -.01 1.00 49 .ll .07 .06 .10 .08 .05 1.00 50 .06 .07 .05 .09 .10 -.02 .28 1.00 51 .12 .05 .10 .03 .06 .04 .17 .16 1.00 52 .04 .08 .05 .10 .06 -.OO .13 .15 .28 1.00 53 .09 .05 .10 .01 .06 -.02 .14 .07 .12 .13 1.00 54 .03 .01 .01 08 .05 .05 .01 .05 .02 .02 .00 1.00 55 .03 .02 .04 .08 .06 .03 .05 .07 .03 .05 .05 .06 1.00 56 .10 .05 .00 .01 .04 .03 .04 .05 .05 .04 .07 .01 .07 1.00 57 .08 .07 .04 .09 .05 .03 .10 .08 .05 .08 .10 .08 .08 .10 1.00 58 .01 .05 .01 .01 .03 .01 .05 .00 .03 .04 .oo .02 .02 .02 .04 1.00 59 .02 .04 .00 .01 .01 .03 .05 .00 .03 .04 .OS .02 .01 .03 .06 .02 1.00 60 .01 .04 .09 .01 -.01 .02 -.OO .03 .00 .01 .02 .03 .02 .01 .00 .04 -.01 1.00 61 .06 .08 .08 .04 .03 .02 .12 .08 .09 .10 .09 .01 .05 .08 .ll .04 .11 -.01 1.00 62 .07 .07 .07 .04 .06 -.00 .05 .09 .09 .09 .08 .03 .06 .03 .03 .03 .06 .07 .15 1.00 63 -.00 .Ol .02 .05 -.OO -.01 .01 .04 .Ol -.01 .00 .03 .02 .02 .01 .00 —.OO .05 .01 .04 1.00 APPENDIX G INTER-SCALE CORRELATIONS 231 4m. m~.u m~.1 a..- no.1 om. mm. mm. .m. me. am. am. am. .e. am. on. em. ~a. No. mm. mm. 6.. Hence m~.1 v..- «4.1 5... am. .4. we. me. me. em. on. 6.. .m. Nm. pm. me. om. on. mm. o.. om. unocouaz< oc..oom on. «N. no. nN.n .~.1 on.1 m~.1 o«.- 4..- 6... .~.1 n~.u o~.1 H«.: .N.. m~.1 .~.1 .~.1 an. n~.1 H«.:oo ucfi.oom H«. m.. H«.: H«.- m~.u ««.- o~.1 m~.u e..- o..- .~.1 w~.u 4... .~.1 mm.. 60.1 .~.1 MN. o«.- ac:m>.om a». a..- a..- a..- o..- .m.u ~..u om.u om.u om.1 H..- m..- m..- ~n.1 a..- m..- on. a..- mcflucsoou.a «6.- h..- mm.1 «6.- mm.: mm.1 6a.- m..- m..- m..- N..- .m.1 w..- m..- ~o.1 .m. o..- mmocnsoflofidnsm on. .6. mm. me. am. .6. no. me. am. .6. .5. Na. .6. pm. me. No. uo>uonno on. co. 66. mm. .4. mm. on. ca. .6. .6. .m. 5.. mm. «a. mo. oucououeH ms. or. on. mm. on. m4. .o. .m. flo. Na. «4. «a. «F. as. HauuoadeH we. m4. on. .6. am. no. 66. as. ~a. mm. .m. as. mm. au.ucmucH :63: cm. ««. mm. mm. mp. «m. .m. as. an. be. me. am. xduuuguonoxum «N. NM. aw. .6. m~. o«. «m. «N. mm. a.. an. ma..un::oo on. «m. co. .m. on. .m. n~. om. mm. mm. HaeuoucH 4m. mm. om. hm. mm. on. as. mm. mm. 26.66: on. no. .5. am. .m. cm. .6. mm. cofiuuosvm me. me. .a. .m. mm. «a. 4.. came .6. .6. .m. .6. .6. pm. 6:066 mm. mm. A». um. om. 6.626» .m. .a. ma. 4.. 0.4: n4. .4. on. ouazo he. om. uuzo¢ m m . H03flfl>HGUCH unmade mumummmmMMmumummmmummwum '3 “a T. s s 3 .4. 5 K m .4. D. n U 0 W T. T U 3. 7.. 1T. 1.... .A m d a a E U. D O .l O D. n a .l T. a a 3.... 9.... P I. I 1 D u. 8 m .4 p. d T. D. .4 1 u an .1". u n o A a .4 I O a U. .4 a A .4 86 6 u u T. a u I. u m T. e .1. .... s a. .4 o D. o n 3 .1. T. o a 8 T. n a .l a a u u M u 8 U 1 .b 8 .D u S a. J 8 T. d 8 .4 .A F .A .o Euom. oamom >ua>wuwmcom o>auoouu< RON anewumaouuou manomuuoucH mo xuoEEsm .0 xficcomm< APPENDIX H SUMMARY OF THE OVERLAP 0F FACTORS AND SUBSCALES (Form D) 232 Summary of the Overlap of Factors and Subscales - A.S.S. Appendix H. oozomno xxx x x xxxxxxx xx xxxx xxxx x x oucououcu xx xxxxx x x x x xx x xx Houuuomem xxxxx xx x xx xx xxx >mouofionu>mm . xxxxxxxxxxxx mawaomcsoo x x x x x Hosanna xxxxx OHUU 5H0”: XXXXXXX Hocozoosom xxxxxxx xxxxxxxx coma x....x x.. x....x x....x x x x....x x x..x x....x x..x x 95.8 xx xxxxxx xxx mausom ..x x..x x.. x x x x xux..x x x v. x x can: xxxxx x xxxx x x xxxxxxxxxx x x UHHflU XXX X XX X X ”.16... x xxxxxxxxxxxxxxxxxxx x xx xxxxxxxxxxxXx uo3ow>uouon x x xx x x x x x xx xxx xxx x x £830 xxx x xx x x x xx x x xx x x x m. 1 97641124489970509979941983246153707814010749 m 22223333344245343344332342244.344354452233333 m . . m t 53773389955122774411116112488466022330000922 w 111111111 2 1112 1 211 2211 22 F m e 13345612345689014.578901345678134690124567012 n1. 11111111222222233333333444445555555666 APPENDIX I COMPUTER PRODUCED SCORING 233 .Amumxomun cw. monoomua Hams» an om3oHHom mafia Eouuon may cw woumfla mum monoom 36H HmOHHflmfim can muoom 36H Hmuoe one ..H x m + m x 4. ha when Imuonu mmz ouoom gucmwao= 3mm one .momqommou .uoouuooofl HOV “mayo ma new .manmummooo m .omuuomonm 6 .mnomoumo =uomwao= map ca .06: on pumnnom mama mflsu mom om .mow Inommumo manomndm man no snow CH mucommmn omwu sumo mo Hones: on» umwa mGEsHoo one .0 EHOMV .m.m.< on» How mcwuoom woosvonm Hmuamfiou .ao.u:. @ c<4uwu as.a:- a ZDmeu .o.c:. 9 >30& «0.06- o homHo ~«.sm. o amsm .n.~:u ac Jake» .0: m.e: m.~ n ”.5: 9.9m N.mn 04nm o.mm none 1.53 n.o¢ «.N: m.nm «.on h.¢c nwom s.cn macaw-» n 0N «N on 0N s a nu cu «m nu 3N a: N 5m 8m kn 4<>Ob an 8 mu m c N n % nW« m «a we : 9N % mu «m: 0 cu m a m o m o o m o m w4w4hcwo < Na 0 a m m «N s« :N m« : oncawuwma ’flwmoc auuhzn hodutn Ion: oxu>ma amzaou tmouzn Ikdcuzh go: :0 w4b2H mxmuau 0N «n maoo onma Hunbzwou mac: n 4<=uu> 92H ova anomu o a Myloam « u zw>uu mthh I. a tack on ugdum >hH>HhHmzwm mtnhomuu< 0| 3 - zomumo mxoz BIBLIOGRAPHY B IBLIOGRAPHY Archer, James Jr. Undergraduates as Paraprofessional Leaders of Interpersonal Communication Skills Train- ing Groups. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Mich- igan State University, 1971. Aronfreed, J. The concept of internalization. In D. Goslin (Ed.), Handbook of Socialization Theory and Research. Chicago: Rand McNally, 1969. Aspy, D. Toward a Technology for Humanizing Education. Champaign, Illinois: Research Press, 1972. Aspy, D. Empathy: Let's get the hell on with it. Counsel— ing Psychologist, 1975, 3, 10-14. Aspy, D. and Roebuck, F. From humane ideas to humane tech- nology and back again many times. Education, Febru- ary, 1975. Astin, H.S. A comparative study of the situational and pre- dictive approaches to the measurement of empathy. Un- published doctoral dissertation, University of Mary- land, 1957. Astin, H.S. Assessment of empathic ability by means of a situational test. Journal of Counseling_Psychology, 1967, 11, 57-60. Avery, A.W. and Danish, S.J. Assessing intertrainer effects on the empathic understanding scale: training the trainer. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 1976, g3, 404-407. Ayres, James. Personal Communication, 1977. Bachrach, H. Adaptive regression, empathy and psychother- apy. Psychotherapy: Theorngesearch and Practice, 1968, 5(4), 203-209. Badrach, H. Empathy: We know what we mean but what do we measure? Archives of General Psychiatry, 1976, g;, 35-38. 234 235 Bachrach, M., Mintz, J., and Luborsky, L. On rating emp- athy and other psychological variables: An exper- ience with the effects of training. gggrnal of Counseling and Clinical Psychology, 1971, §§, 445. Baker, F.B. & Martin, T.J. Fortsp: A Fortrsg Test Analy: sis Packags (Revised for use on the CDC 3600 and'CDC 6500 at Michigan State University by David J. Wright). East Lansing, Michigan: Office of Research Consul- tation, College of Education, 1970. Banks, 6., Bevenson, B., and Carkhuff, R. The effects of counselor race and training upon the counseling pro- cess with Negro clients in initial interviews. Jour- nal of Clinical Psychology, 1966, gs, 70-72. Barrett-Lennard, G.T. Dimensions of therapist response as causal factors in therapeutic change. Psychological Monographs, 1962, 76(43, Whole No. 562). Bell, G.B. and Stopler, R. An attempt at validation of the Empathy Test. Journal of Applied Psychology, 1955, 33, 442-443. Bender, I.E., and Hastorf, A.H. The perceptions of persons: forecasting another person's responses on three per- sonality scales. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psy- chology, 1950, 15, 556-561. Bender, I.E., and Hastorf, A.H. On measuring generalized empathic ability (social sensitivity). Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1953, fig, 503-506. Bergin, A.E. and Jasper, L.G. Correlates of empathy in psychotherapy: A replication. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 1969, 11, 477-481. Bergin, A.E. and Strupp, H. New directives in psychotherapy research. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 1970, 16, 13-26 0 Bergin, A.E. and Suinn, R.M. Individual psychotherapy and behavior change. Annual Review of Psychology, 1975, gg, 509-556. , Beutler, L.E., Johnson, D.T., Neville Jr., C.W. and WOrkman, S.N. ”Accurate empathy" and the A-B dichotomy. Jour- nal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 1972, gs, 372-375. Blass, D.D. and Heck, E.J. Accuracy of accuracy empathy ratings. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 1975, 22 243-246. 236 Borke, H. Interpersonal perception of young children: ‘ Egocentrism or empathy? Developmental Psychology, 1971’ E, 263-2690 Borke, H. The development of empathy in Chinese and Amer- ican children between three and six years of age: A cross-cultural study. Developmental Psychology, 1973, 2, 102-108. Borke, H. Chandler and Greenspan's "erstaz egocentrism": A rejoinder. Developmental Psychology, 1973, g, 107- 109. Brunclik, H., Thruston, J.B. and Feldhusen, J. The Empathy Inventory. Nursing Outlook, 1967, 15, 41-44. Buchheimer, A. The development of ideas about empathy. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 1963, 19, 61-70. Buchheimer, A., Goodman, J. and Sircus, G. Vidiotapes and kinescopic recordings as situational test and labor- atory exercises in empathy for the training of coun- selors. (Technical Report to the USOE, NDEA of 1958, Title VII, Research Project 7-42-055041670) New York: Hunter College, City University of New York, 1965. Burns, N. and Cavey, L. Age differences in empathic ability among children. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 1957, 11, 227-230. Buros, O.K. The sixth mental measurements yearbook. High- land Park, New Jersey: Gryphon, 1965. Burstein, J.W., and Carkhuff, R.R. Objective therapist and client ratings of therapist offered facilitative con- ditions of moderate to low functioning therapists. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 1968, 24, 240-249. Campbell, R.C. Development and validation of a multiple- choice scale to measure affective sensitivity (em- pathy). Dissertation Abstracts, 1968, 28(10-A), 3967-3968. Campbell, R.J., Kagan, N. and Krathwohl, D.R. The develop- ment of a scale to measure affective sensitivity (empathy). Journal of Counselinngsychology, 1971, lg, 407-412. Caracina, P.F. and Vicory, J.R. Correlates of phenomenolog- ical and judged empathy. Journal of Counseling Psy- ChOlOSZ, 1969' kg, 510-515. 237 Carkhuff, R.R. Helping and Human Relations. VOls. I and II. New York: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston, 1969. Carkhuff, R.R. The development of systematic human resource development models. CounselingyPsyghologist, 1972, 3' 4-100 Chambers, F.M. Empathy and scholastic success. Personal and Guidance Journal, 1957, 3g, 282-284. Chandler, D.R. Construct validity of a scale of accurate empathy in psychotherapists. Dissertstion Abstracts International, 1971, 32(3-B), 1836-1837. Chapman, J.L. The development and validation of a scale to measure empathy. (Doctoral dissertation, Michigan State University) Ann Arbor, Michigan, University Microfilms, 1966, No. 66-14, 108. Chapman, J.L. and Kagan, N. Development and validation of a scale to measure empathy. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 1971, 18, 281-282. Chinsky, J.M. and Rappaport, J. Brief critique of the mean- ing and reliability of "accurate empathy" ratings. Psychological Bulletin, 1970, 1;, 379-382. Chournos, G. Pretraining reactions to a recorded interview as a means of screening counselor trainers. Disser- gstion Abstracts International, 1970, 30(12-B), 5685. Chronback, L.J. Coefficient alpha and the internal struc- ture of tests. Psychometrika, 1951, 16, 297-334. Collingwood, T., Hefele, T., Muehlberg, N. and Drasgow, J. Toward identification of the therapeutic facilita- tion factor. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 1970, 36, 118-120. Cowden, R.C. Empathy or projection. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 1955, 11, 188-190. Craddick, R.A., Leipold, W.C. and Leipold, V. Effect of role-empathy on height of human figures drawn by male alcoholics. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 1970, 39, 747,752. Davidoff, M.D. A study of empathy and correlations of prej- udice toward a minority group. Purdue Uniyersity Studies in Higher Education, 1949, 61, 1-67. 238 Dendy, R.F. A model for the training of undergraduate res- idence hall assistance as paraprofessional counselors using vidiotape techniques and Interpersonal Process Recall (IPR). Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Michigan State University, 1971. Deutsch, F. The effects of sex of subject and story charac- ter on preschoolers' perceptions of affective re- sponses and intrapersonal behavior in story sequences. Developmental Psychology, 1975, 11, 112-113. Diskin, P.A. A study of predictive empathy and the ability of student teachers to maintain harmonious interper- sonal relations in selected elementary classrooms. Dissertation Abstracts, 1956, 16, 1399. Dixon, W.R. and Morse, W.C. The prediction of teacher per- formances: Empathic potential. Journal of Teacher Dymond, Rosalind F. "A Preliminary Investigation of the Relation of Insight and Empathy." Journal of Con- sulting Psychology, 1948, 11, 228-233. Dymond, R.R. A scale for the measurement of empathic abil- ity. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 1949, 11, 127-133. ' Dymond, R.R. Personality and empathy. Journal of Consult- ing Psychology, 1950, 11, 343-350. Fenichel, O. The Psychoanalyt1gTheory of Neurosis, New York: W.W. Norton, 1945. Feshbach, N.D. Empathy in children: some theoretical and empirical considerations. The Counseling Psycholo- SiSt' 1975' 2' 25-30. Feshbach, N. and Feshbach, S. The relationship between em- pathy and aggression in two age groups. Development- al Psychology, 1969, 1, 102-107. Feshbach, N.D. and Roe, K. Empathy in six- and seven-year- olds. Child Development, 1968, 11, 133-145. Fromm-Reichman, F. Princip1es of intensivespsychgtherapy. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1950. Gates, G.S. An experimental study of the growth of percep- tion. Journal of Educational Psychology, 1923, 11, 449-461. 239 Gladstein, G.A. Empathy and counseling outcome: An empir- ical and conceptual review. The CounselingsPsychol- Goodman, N. and Ofshe, R. Empathy, communication efficiency, and marital status. Journal of Marriage and the Fam- ily, 1968, 11 597-603. - Gordon, D. A device for demonstrating empathy. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1934, 11, 892-893. Gray, 3., Nida, R.A. and Coonfield, T.J. Empathic Listening Test: An instrument for the selection and training of telephone crisis workers. Journal of Communigy Psychology, 1976, 1, 199-205. Guiora, A.Z. On clinical diagnosis and prediction. Psy- chological Reports, 1965, 11, 779-784. Guiora, A.Z. Toward a systematic study of empathy. Com- pgehensive Psychiatry, 1967, 1, 375-385. Halkides, G. An experimental study of four conditions nec- essary for therapeutic change. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Chicago, 1958. Halpern, H.M. Empathy, similarity, and self-satisfaction.‘ Journal of Consulting Psychology, 1955, 11, 449-452. Hansen, J.C., Noore, G.D.,and Carkhuff, R. The differen- tial relationship of objective and client percep- tions of counseling. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 1968, 11, 244-246. Hastorf, A.H. and Bender, I.E. A caution respecting the measurement of empathic ability. Journal of Abnor- mal and Social Psychology, 1952, 11, 574-576. Hawkes, G.R. and R.L. Egbert. "Personal Values and the Empathic Response: Their Inter-Relationships." Journal of Educational Psychology, 1954, 11, 469- 476. Hefele, T. and Hurst, M.W. Interpersonal skill measurement: Prediction, validity and utility. The Counseling Psycho1ogist, 1972, 1, 62-69. Helfand, I. Role playing in schzophrenia: A study of empathy. Dissertation Abstracts, 1955, 11, 1117. Hogan, R. Development of an empathy scale. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 1969, 33(3), 307-316. 240 Hogan, R. Empathy: A conceptual and Psychometric analysis. The Counseling Psychologist, 1975, 1, 14-18. Hill, M.N. Suicidal behavior in adolescents and its rela- tionship to lack of parental empathy. Dissertation Abstracts International, 1970, 31(l-A), 472. Hurst, M.W. An investigation of empathy and its measure- ment: The development of an integrative systems model. Dissertation Abstracts International, 1974. Iannotti, R.J. The nature and measurement of empathy in children. The Counseling Psychologist, 1975, 1, 21-25. Ingram, Barabara. Defensive Style and Affective Sensitivity. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Southern California, 1977. Irving, S.G. Parental empathy and_adolescent adjustment. Dissertation Abstracts International, 1966, 27(3-B), 967-968. Jackson, W. and A.C. Carr. "Empathic Ability in Normals and Schizophrenics." gournal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1955, 11, 79-82. Jarrard, L. Empathy: The concept and its industrial appli- cations. Personal Psychology, 1956, 1, 157-167. Jones, J.E. Helping relationship response tendencies and occupational affiliation. Personnel and Guidance Journal, 1967, 671-675. Jung, C.G. Psychological Types. London: Rutledge and Ke- gan Paul, 1923. [New translation, Princeton-Boll- ingen Press. Volume 6 of complete works].' Kagan, N. Personal Communication, 1977. Kagan, N., Krathwohl, D. R. and Farqurhar, W. W. A progress report: Interpersonal process recall technique. Submitted under Title VII to U. S. Office of Educa- tion, Washington, D. C., 1964. Kagan, N., Krathwohl, D.R and Farquhar, W.W. Interpersonal process recall: Stimulated recall by videotape in exploratory studies of counseling and teaching-learn- ing-final report, (NDEA Title VII, Grant No. OE 7-32- 0410-216) East Lansing: Michigan State University, 1965. 241 Kagan, N., Drathwohl, D., Goldberg, A., Campbell, R., chauble, P., Greenberg, 3., Danish, S., Resnikoff, A., Bowes, J. and Bondy, S. Studies in human inter- action: Interpersonal process recall stimulated by videotape. (Final Report RR-20, United States De- partment of Health, Education and Welfare). East Lansing: Michigan State University, Educational Pub- lication Services, 1967. Kagan, N. and Schneider, J. Affective Sensitivity Scale: Form D, Mason Michigan: Mason Media, Inc., 1975. Kagan, N. Schneider, J. and Werner D. Toward the creation of reliable, valid and practical measures of inter— personal sensitivity: The affective sensitivity scale. Paper presented American Psychological Asso- ciation, San Francisco, August, 1977. Kalisch, B.J. An experiment in the development of inter- active and predictive empathy in nursing students. Dissertation Abstracts International, 1971, 31(11-B), 6715. ‘—‘——‘_‘ Kerr, W.A. The Empathy Test, Chicago: Psychometric Affil- iates, 1947. Kerr, W.A. and Speroff, B.J. Validation and evaluation of the empathy test. Journal of General Psychology, Kurtz, R.R. A comparison of different approaches to the measurement of counselor empathy in personal coun- seling. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Michigan State University, 1970. Kurtz, R.R. and Grummon, D.L. Different approaches to the measurement of therapist empathy and their relation- ship to therapy outcomes. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 1972, 11, 106-115. Lambert, M.J. and DeJulio Outcome research in Carkhuff's Human Resource Development Training Programs: Where is the donut? The Counselinngsychologist, 1977, 1, 79-86. Lindgren, H.E. and Robinson, J. Evaluation of Dymond's test of insight and empathy. Journal of Consultisg Psychology, 1953, 11, 172-176. Lord, F.M. and Novick, M.R. Statistical theories of mental test scores. Reading, Mass.: Addison-Wesley, 1968. 242 Luborsky, L., Chandler, M., Auerbach, A., et a1. Factors influencing the outcome of therapy: A review of quantitative research. Psychological Bulletin, 1971, 11, 145-185. Mohoney, S.C. The literature empathy test: Development of a procedure for differentiating behavior "good empa- thizers" and "poor empathizers." Dissertation Ab- stracts, 1960, 11, 674. McCaulley, N.H. Description of the Myers-Briggs Type In- dicator, Unpublished manuscript, University of Flor- ida, 1971. McNally, H. and Drummond, R. Ratings of Carkhuff's facil- itative conditions: A second look. gounselor Edu- cation and Sgpervision, 1974, 11, 73-75. Milgram, N.A. Cognitive and empathic factors in role-taking by schizophrenic and brain damaged patients. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1960, 11, 219-224. Mitchell, R.M., Truax, C.B., Bozarth, J.D. and Kraft, C.C. Antecedents to psychological outcome. NIMH Final Report - MB 12306, 1973. Mood, D., Johnson, J and Shantz, C.U. Young children's understanding of the affective states of others. Technical Report No. 14, Wayne State University, Muehlberg, N., Pierce, R. and Drasgow, J. A factor analysis of therapeutically facilitative conditions. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 1969, 11, 93-95. Mullen, J. and Abelis, N. Relationship of liking, empathy, and therapist's experience to outcome of therapy. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 1971, 11, 39-43. Murstein, 3.1. Some comments on the measurement of projec- tion and empathy. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 1957, 11, 81-82. Myers, I.B. Manual: The Myers-Briggs Type Indigator, New Jersey: EducationaI_Testing Service, 1962. Nie, N.H., Hull, C.H., Jenkins, J.G., Steinbrenner, K. and Bent, D.H. Statistical Package for the Social Sci- ences, 2nd Edition, New York: McGraw-Hill, 1975. O'Hern, J.S. A study of sensitivity as a measurable concept for the screening and evaluation of counselors. Un- published doctoral dissertation. Boston University, 1962. 243 O'Hern, J.S. and Arbuckle, D.S. Sensitivity: A measureable concept? The Personnel and Guidance Journa1, 1964, 11, 572-576. Partyka, L.B. Two aspects of empathic awareness in young children: Affective and cognitive role-flaking. Un- published doctoral disserations, Michigan*State Uni- versity, 1974. Pfeiffer, J.W. and Jones, J.E. The 1973 Annual Handbook for Groueracilitators, Iowa City, Iowa: UHiVersity Associates, 1973. Porter, E.H. An Introduction to therapeutic counseling. Boston: Houghton-Mifflin, 1950. Rank, R.C. Counseling Competence and Perceptions. The Personnel and Guidance Journal, 1966, 11, 359-365. Rappaport, J. and Chimsky, J.M. Accurate empathy: Confu- sion of a construct. Psychological Bulletin, 1972, 11, 400-404. Reid, D.K. and W.V. Snyder. "Experiment on 'Recognition of Feeling' in Non-directive Psychotherapy." Journal of Clinical Psychology, 1947, 1, 128-135. Resnikoff, A. A measure of discrimination for the predic- tion of counselor effects. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 1972, 11, 464-465. Rogers, C.R. Client-ceasgred Therapy. Boston: Houghton- Mifflin Company, 1951. Rogers, C.R. On Becoming a Person. Boston: Houghton- Mifflin, 1961. Rogers, C.R. (ED.) The therapeutic relationsh1p and its impact: A study of_psychotherapy with schizophren- ics. Madison: University of Wisconsin Press, 1967. Rogers, C.R. Empathic: An unappreciated way of being. Counseling Psychologist, 1975, 1, 2-10. Roseman, M.F. and Drenner, W.T. Empathy in schizophrenics, neurotics, and normals. Psychological Reporgs, 1967, 11, 863-864. Rowe, K.K. A 50-hour intensified IPR training program for counselors. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Michigan State University, 1972. 244 Sapolsky, A. Effect of interpersonal relationships upon verbal conditioning. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1960, 11, 241-246. Schmidt, W.H. and Scheifley, V. Computer Programs for Max- imum Likelihood Factor Analysis - An Adaptation and Modification of K.G. Joreskogis Programs For Use on the Michigan State UniversiEy CDC 6500 Computer. East Lansing, Michigan: Michigan State University,- Center for Urban Afairs, 1970. Schneider, J. Personal communication, 1977. Schneider, J., Kagan, N. and Werner, D. The development of a Measure of empathy: The Affective Sensitivity Scale. Paper presented at American Psychological Association, San Francisco, August, 1977. Scott, W.A. Attitude measurement. In G. Lindzey and E. Aronson (Eds.) The Handbook of Social Psychology. Reading Mass.: Addison-Wesley, 1968f Shantz, C.U. Empathy in relation to social cognitive de- velopment. Counseling Psycholog1st, 1975, 1, 18-21. Selfridge, P.F. Effects of in-service training in inter- personal communication skills on school counselors. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, State University of New York at Albany, 1972. Shelton, J.L. The relationship of Various levels of accur- ate empathy to rehabilitation outcome as measured by the Interaction scale. Unpublished doctoral disser- tation, University of Utah, 1969. Speroff, B.J. Empathy and role reversal as factors in in- dustrial harmony. Journal of Social Psychology, Stewart, D.A. "Psychogenesis of Empathy." Psychoanalytic Review, 1954, 11, 216-228. ' Stotland, E. and Walsh, J. Birth order, in an experimental study of empathy. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1963, 11, 610-614. Sullivan, H.S. Conceptions of Modern Psychiatry, Washing- ton, D.C.: William Alanson White Psychiatric Foun- dation, 1947. Sydnor, G.L., Abridge, R.L., and Parkhill, N.L. Human Re- lations: A Manual for Trainers. Minden, La. Human Resources DevElopment Training Institute, 1973. 245 Tausch, R., Bastine, R., Bommert, H., Minsel, W.R. and Nickel, H. Weitere Untersuchung der Auswirkung unter Prozesse klientenzentrierter Gesprachpsycho- therapie. Zeitscgrift fur Klinische Psychologie, 1972, 1, 1, 232-250. Tausch, R., Bastine, R., Friese, H. and Sander, K. Vari- ablen und Ergebnisse bei Psychotherapie mit alter- nieranden Psychotherapeuten. Verlog fur Psychologie, 1970, 11, 1-34. Tobolski, F.D. and Kerr, W. Predictive value of the Em— Truax, Truax, Truax, Truax, Truax, pathy Test in automobile salesmanship. Journal of Applied Psychology, 1952, 11, 310-311. C.B. Toward a tentative measurement of central ther- apeutic ingredients. Arkansas Rehabilitation Re- search and Training Center, University of Arkansas, 1961. C.B. Therapist empathy, warmth, and genuineness and patient personality change in group psychotherapy: A comparison of unit measures, time, sample measures, patient perception measures. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 1966, 11, 225-229. C. and Carkhuff, R.R. Toward Effective Counselipg and Psychotherapy. Chicago: Aldine, 1967. C. and Mitchell, K.M. Research on certain therapist interpersonal skills in relations to process and out- come. In Bergen, A.E. and Garfield, S.L. (Eds.) Handbook of Psychotherapy and Behavior Change: An Empirical Ana1ysis. New York: John Wiley and Sons, Inc., 1971) 229-344. C. and Tatum, C.R. An extension from the effective psychotherapeutic model to constructive personality change in preschool children. Childhood Education, 1966 3 fly 456-462. Van Zelst, R.H. Empathy scores of union leaders. Journal of Applied Psychology, 1952, 11, 293-295. Wallace, J. An abilities conception of personality: Some implications for personality measurement. American PEXChOIQQiSt' 1966’ El, 132-138. Wallston, K.A. and Weitz, L.J. Measurement of the core di- mensions of helping. Journal of Counseling Psychol- 931' 1975' £3, 567-5690 246 Walton, W.E. Empathic responses in children. Ps chol. Monogr. 48/213: 40-67(1936). Psychologicai Mbno- graphs, 1936, 48/213, 40-67. Weinstein, E.A. The development of interpersonal compe- tence. In D. Goslin (Ed.), Handbook of Socializa- tion Theory and Research. Chicago: Rand McNail , 1969. - Weiss, J.H. Effect of professional training and amount and accuracy of information on behavioral predictions. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 1963, 11, 257-262. Weiss, R.L., Krasner, L. and Ullmann, L.P. Responsivity to verbal conditioning as a function of emotional atmos- phere and patterning of reinforcement. Psychological Reports, 1960, 1, 415-426. Werner, D.W., Kagan, N. and Schneider, J. The measurement of Affective Sensitivity: The development of an in- strument. Paper presented at 16th Annual Conference on Medical Education, Washington D.C., 1977. Zimmerman, I.L. and Woo-Sam, J.M. Clinical Interpretation of the Wechsler Adult Igtelligence Scale, New York: Grune and Strattion, 1973.