.. 1!] {ff .1. t. .5. avejflwwfigh .. . .r '55: 11:...» “us I U.‘".‘IA ’1 0",’ 3:11.: .3... . . .w f.n.,..)za. H... . 2.9.2.. :5- . navtxtv- .L v 13. .2": a . n,:‘ ‘A l .n....... .5... .32 fit .31... .v.Lax«..$..\J'VL » 2v: .\.q.a..aa. .V. , Dara... t r- 032“... 7.. if“. .33. c1793... :1 3 I... “Pun farf‘ . . 0 VI '3 "H y a .«r‘ 6...... .. ~ .v _. . . . .112! K: . :0; :1. 3px;} 1:151:33 . . 1.: vH‘J‘OAl‘i 5 ~ . A in: y 2.9. .. I . t": “Jug“ '53.“ v’l' n' > ' 3."? I). . \to I: ; c . p . r ynv i I . o. 1.1:}? hie...- 1‘“ :x.:. . .fmnfixvtvii v!!! 71%. .- v... . a II M ' 3:1,!)- “5w :1 . ‘ n:v ‘ a , . ‘ \f’f)- . . . .. a... . {fix , vhn‘oltvfi - . r “H. , , .. - . .. . . tifif- -3. X .. . . 3 .3. its»! _ 31...... {3.9.5-5. . z 3.13.. .i F. i r1231? 1 ii .-,;. artiilz: . .333... . 21...}: , l. : §I§.FVA o 7.... ‘11..qu 1.5.5: .I...u(,..~.¥... 1p...;sl:aav . . IA. L. In Ha I:- 1.7%.: . . . x51. :. .v 1. :5... o . . :2. , .. .1 .93.. a... ... a...» me... an... - 35:... «ivy»... we 5. . .1041... .1. _. . 5:39:37 5:... .r i i. 1. . .. 2.72.2:71. 3:10;...“b. .i....a¢§vl '1‘ . . ab . . . hymn.» J , . . Pkg... , , . . . u 1.... a . . I v... . . . valna¢bwnf 2 fin! . vagrflnnflxia citvbaocfiv v . . . if. . is...3.5.3....32?! exist <9: .. I ... . .fvunflreat .fiv‘. hunk... ufiuqifivfifi .5», ,5. V . «in! .02)....an 3!... 3 a. . 1‘11? {$.11pr , Aquila.» at} 154.. iiivis; £339.! .11 up... up Ct: \ u 7 . . . 1 ... 9!. , , , . o. 5......39. I...» 53.3.: :zflrnn . . 1.. . xylvflndz . 1‘. . .503”. it‘s... .5..." .A .ziuziiiyiail. (lb I I r V I .p r: ...v . . .5 :35». . 1...". , z 3.... . 9:39;:- .v (7‘! a»: x r. : 00.....3439 .1 two...ova....p..f .. . u:..n.y..onxi LI...- . ’ rI'Ii\("r£.v-l-VU&IA '2 l I v .5 ‘6 ’k 3%: «if: *i v 5.. n ‘ .. ..~quvI.| L74 3...... . .1... IX , ' 3 .,. .Vl‘ ‘ '3 0 .m ab. :1) *w u ,A o u v . . h. . . I! . - 51.915292: .. o . . . .1 v)..av...a!’!.56.)l I X Ctr A 0‘ . . . Qvlvv 'L'II'A ‘III 2 i . , . . Xv XII-6.. 5933313.}... . . x 25!... .v: . . . . jut”. . (any)! 0...! v1.4. 1 V “VIA" Ain't; 3W4”. fxfll‘ : 5.1.59.1}; S v . . . . anti.) hi.» . l'trly£'l_ , 1391.31; ¢ alavba. vrlvyzvnthrlo a (.1311, O 0.1 . 5.7. .3)? I 5%! . 1.. ‘ . invisuf ICC ‘ IIIWAOILri ., . it! .1. . .2 .52).! .5 . 2-33;... V .. . . (1.15.: $95.13!»... .a.r.....3..})v.....$!t.l.. La..ln.v.o.¢3v..ri3£. 4.5. 5.. 5.1% . f 3:: . 1:3,...” (FF: :2 13-13:}! . .91.... $13.2”, 5912.536: . 33"}; $513!. 1; .9D-I f: . 1.. .vk... ~v.‘ .mQ-Jvh. .3 a... . k. n , . . . 3000 ll)! . outin- .I.l}oplb’1‘l}bla>ruvr .5)!" (it... 1.. . .tbOO-t Iv!- ‘ ‘ , -34.? . . »-\va.\b .xyA . . .ii.‘ . . . . . . . )D 2.1“}! , . . . : : viilfliiu.‘ Aoavl. .‘xvlwrt I . (at): 0‘0? . till|l.-‘ll'nl rA-iJa." nix.) » . ¢ y. L . twin is». p L. . ,1... - rolirlnflh. . r. . ‘1} cn'l ‘1' 3.13,!!! . .- .o...$.. «a 3.5.1.) 1.7? .5063. {s37 ‘ ‘\ .{v- ‘.tlv.: . . {33... :5. }. I .. . 40..» . . .110: bill: t....v.[. v. i. IL. I. . kfllhvhltttur ‘9!»yuavruie .v..‘:~ .Dlvr Ir‘aal.o. . .r :$.«..Ioahzvli.|il .r. A 4 .l. r . 215?: ..u!o}t.brrt!’!sb‘avtl . ’r’fle . . v; I 1.9.)! . . -01.)... II: .9 ,LLL. .....D«,. THESiS SITY LIBRARI ISE lllllllll l H I ll 31293 This is to certify that the thesis entitled THE ROLE OF PROTECTIVE FACTORS IN THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN RISK AND CHILD EXTERNALIZING BEHAVIOR PROBLEMS OVER A CONTINUUM OF RISK LEVELS presented by Sondra R. Wilen has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for M.A. Psychology degree in QV/WPJF/«VZ Major prgj/ essor Dan: November 18, 1997 0-7 639 MSU is an Affirmative Action/Equal Opportunity Institution LIBRARY University Michigan State PLACE IN RETURN BOX to remove this checkout from your record. TO AVOID FINES return on or before date due. DATE DUE DATE DUE DATE DUE MAGIC 2 affirms .mulzm@ q ‘6‘ . Aug 31 g 2222 p v 1/98 CJCIHC/DataDue.p&5-p.14 THE ROLE OF PROTECTIVE FACTORS IN THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN RISK AND CHILD EXTERNALIZING BEHAVIOR PROBLEMS OVER A CONTINUUM OF RISK LEVELS By Sondra R Wilen A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF ARTS Department of Psychology 1997 ABSTRACT THE ROLE OF PROTECTIVE FACTORS IN THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN RISK AND CHILD EXTERNALIZING BEHAVIOR PROBLEMS OVER A CONTINUUM OF RISK LEVELS By Sondra R. Wilen This longitudinal study examined the efl‘ects of multiple risk and protective factors over a continuum of risk levels on the development of externalizing behavior problems in a sample of population-based preschool-aged children of alcoholics and a contrast sample of children from non-alcoholic families. The results of this study indicate that risk and protective factors at time I predicted child behavior problems three years later. Child temperament moderated the efi‘ects of paternal antisociality and maternal alcoholism on child behavior problems, and maternal warmth and family cohesion mediated the effects of maternal depression. The relationship between risk level and child behavior problems was found to change at a cumulative risk level of two, with the individual risk factors being predictive of child outcome in the high risk but not the low risk group. Similarly, risk and protective factors were found to be more negatively correlated in the high risk group than in the low risk group, and high levels of protection were found to be unlikely within an environment characterized by high risk. The importance of examining the relationship between risk, protection, and child outcome over a continuum of risk is discussed. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS I would like to thank Dr. Hiram Fitzgerald for taking on the responsibility of chairing my thesis committee. His knowledge of the literature and enthusiasm for the associated project constituted a valuable source of information and inspiration. Dr. Fitzgerald’s feedback as this work transformed was vital, as were his support and guidance through its complexities. Similarly, Dr. Robert Zucker’s ambition in posing fascinating research problems and brilliance in conceptualizing the core issues were instrumental to the completion of this work as it stands in its present form. Dr. Zucker has taught me much about how to conceive a problem and what tactics to employ in creating a solution — invaluable skills I will carry with me throughout my career. To Dr. Alytia Levendoskyl owe a tremendous debt of gratitude for her involvement and continued support during the entire thesis process. Dr. Levendosky was a crucial source of structure, encouragement, and feedback. She is a true mentor and has served as a model for growth as a professional woman. I also thank Dr. Alex VonEye and Dr. Rick DeShon for the generous giving of their time and willingness to share their remarkable statistical knowledge. Without their assistance, this work would simply not have been possible. Dr. Anne Bogat was willing to ofl‘er words of wisdom in guidance toward completion both of this project and the program in general. Her door has always been open to me, and for that I am grateful. «mi-ammo- I would also like to thank three very special friends. To Michael Semel I owe endless thanks for his faith in my abilities, patience, and willingness to impart the wisdom he has acquired. To Lisa Galasso, words cannot express my thanks for the countless times she lent a kind ear and a supportive word. To Jennifer McQueeney, despite her distance in body, her support in heart and mind was priceless. To my husband and best fiiend, Brian, I extend my heartfelt thanks for his love, patience, and support throughout both this endeavor and my life as a whole. Brian provided endless guidance in shaping my style of writing. He has shown never-ending faith in me, and I am grateful he has chosen to share my life. Finally, I thank the many families who participated in this project. Their honesty and generosity with their time has been priceless to this work. I especially thank the children and wish them nothing but the best in their continual struggle to grow. iv TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES ................................................................................ vii-viii LIST OF FIGURES .................................................................................... ix INTRODUCTION ...................................................................................... 1 RISK FACTORS AND CHILD OUTCOME ........................................................ 3 Parental Psychopathology ...................................................................... 3 Co-occurring Parental Diagnoses .................................................... 6 Environmental Risk Factors .................................................................... 8 Multiple Risk Factors ......................................................................... 10 PROTECTIVE FACTORS AND CHILD OUTCOME .......................................... 14 Child Characteristics .......................................................................... 17 Intelligence ............................................................................ l7 Temperament ......................................................................... 19 Family Environment ........................................................................... 21 Family Cohesion ...................................................................... 21 Home Environment .................................................................. 23 Maternal Warmth ..................................................................... 25 RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN RISK AND PROTECTIVE FACTORS ....................... 27 Orthogonality of Constructs ................................................................ 27 Implications of Sample Variability .......................................................... 29 HYPOTHESES ......................................................................................... 33 Predicted Main Effects ........................................................................ 34 Predicted Moderating Efl‘ects ................................................................ 35 Predicted Mediating Effects .................................................................. 35 Predicted Efl‘ects of Risk Level .............................................................. 36 METHODS ............................................................................................. 37 Subjects .......................................................................................... 37 Procedures ...................................................................................... 39 Measures ....................................................................................... 39 Measures of Risk ..................................................................... 40 Measures of Child Protection ....................................................... 44 V Measures of Family Environment Protection ..................................... 45 Measure of Child Outcome ......................................................... 47 RESULTS ............................................................................................... 48 Missing Data and Outliers ................................................................... 48 Descriptive Statistics .......................................................................... 48 Multiple Regression Analyses ............................................................... 48 Main Efl‘ects ........................................................................... 48 Moderating Effects ................................................................... 51 Mediating Efl‘ects ..................................................................... 55 Efl‘ects of Risk Level ................................................................. 57 DISCUSSION .......................................................................................... 64 Main Efl‘ects of Risk Factors ................................................................. 65 Main Efl‘ects of Protective Factors .......................................................... 68 Moderating Effects of Child Characteristics ............................................... 69 Mediating Efl‘ects of Family Environment Characteristics ............................. 71 Efi‘ects of Risk Level .......................................................................... 73 Limitations of the Current Study and Directions for Future Research ................. 79 APPENDIX A TABLES .................................... .. ............................................................ 82 APPENDIX B FIGURES ................................................................................................ 95 REFERENCES ....................................................................................... 102 vi LIST OF TABLES Table l - Descriptive Statistics for Risk Variables, Protective Variables, and Extemalizing Behavior Problems .................................................................. 82 Table 2 - Summary of Multiple Regression Analyses Predicting Child Extemalizing Behavior Problems (n=214): Main Efl‘ects of Risk Factors and Protective Factors. . .83 Table 3 - Summary of Multiple Regression Analysis Predicting Family Cohesion (n=214): Main Effects of Risk Factors ....................................................... 84 Table 4 - Summary of Multiple Regression Analysis Predicting Stimulating Home Environment (n=214): Main Efl‘ects of Risk Factors ............................ 85 Table 5 - Summary of Multiple Regression Analysis Predicting Maternal Warmth (n=214): Main Effects of Risk Factors ....................................................... 86 Table 6 - Summary of Hierarchical Regression Analyses for Variables Predicting Child Extemalizing Behavior Problems (n=214): Moderating Efi‘ects of Child Protection ............................................................................. 87 Table 7 - Summary of Hierarchical Regression Analyses for Variables Predicting Child Extemalizing Behavior Problems (n=214): Mediating Efl‘ects of F arnily Environment Protection ............................................................. 88 Table 8 - Summary of the Relationships Between Risk and a High Level of Child Extemalizing Behavior Problems Across Risk Level Subpopulations ........ 89 Table 9 - Summary of Multiple Regression Analyses Predicting Child Extemalizing Behavior Problems: Main Efl‘ects of Risk Factors Within Risk Level Subgroups ..................................................................... 90 Table 10- Intercorrelations Between Individual Risk and Protective Variables Within Risk Level Subgroups ..................................................................... 91 Table 11- Summary of the Relationships Between Individual Risk and Protective Variables Across Risk Level Subpopulations ................................................ 92 Table 12- Summary of Multiple Regression Analyses Predicting Child Reactivity: Main Efi‘ects of Risk Factors Within Risk Level Subgroups ........................ 93 Table 13- Summary of Multiple Regression Analyses Predicting Family Cohesion: Main Efl‘ects of Risk Factors Within Risk Level Subgroups ........................ 94 LIST OF FIGURES Figure l - Model of Hypothesized Relationships Between Risk, Protection, and Child Outcome .............................................................................. 95 Figure 2 - Hypothesized Relationship Between Risk and Extemalizing Behavior Problems Over the Continuum of Risk Levels ............................................... 96 Figure 3 - Hypothesized Inverse Relationship Between Risk and Protection Over the Continuum of Risk Levels ............................................... 97 Figure 4- Moderating Effect of Child Reactivity on the Relationship Between Maternal Alcoholism and Child Extemalizing Behavior Problems ............ 98 Figure 5 - Moderating Efl‘ect of Child Attention on the Relationship Between Paternal Antisociality and Child Extemalizing Behavior Problems ............ 99 Figure 6- Moderating Efl‘ect of Child Approach-Mthdrawal/Adaptability on the Relationship Between Maternal Alcoholism and Child Extemalizing Behavior Problems ........ . ..................................................................... 100 Figure 7- Estimation of Breakpoint in Slope of the Regression Equation Predicting Extemalizing Behavior Problems .................................... 101 ix INTRODUCTION Research has consistently indicated that children of alcoholics are at a higher risk than the general population for the development of a variety of behavior problems in childhood (e. g. Fitzgerald et al., 1993; Zucker & Fitzgerald, 1991; Jansen, Fitzgerald, Ham, & Zucker, 1995; Hill & Muka, 1996; West & Prinz 1987; Werner, 1986). However, studies also have consistently found that a substantial number of children of alcoholics never develop problems despite evidence of having been placed at an increased risk. As a result, many researchers have attempted to determine what difl‘erentiates those children of alcoholics who experience dimculties from those who are resilient (e.g., Fitzgerald et al., 1993, Werner, 1986). Although this area has been studied extensively, an understanding of what protects some children of alcoholics from poor outcOmes has not yet been fully achieved; this inadequacy of understanding has impeded preventive efl‘orts. Much of the previous research has utilized retrospective or cross-sectional designs with samples which do not include children at the highest end of the continuum of risk and therefore cannot adequately address the role of protective child characteristics and the family environment as moderators or mediators of the development of problems in children of alcoholics (Rogosch, Chassin, & Sher, 1990; Sher & Trull, 1996). While functionally more attractive to researchers, it does not appear that these methods can successfully explain the complex 2 relationships between multiple risk factors and protective intervening variables which result in difl‘erential outcomes in children of alcoholics. The purpose of this study is to examine the relationship between multiple risk and protective factors and the role of these variables over time in the development of externalizing behavior problems in children. The current investigation hypothesizes that child characteristics, such as intelligence and temperament, as well as aspects of the family environment, such as family cohesion and maternal warmth, will mitigate the adverse impact of parental psychopathology and environmental risk factors on'child development. This investigation firrther hypothesizes that there exists a difl‘erential relationship between risk and protection among risk status subpopulations, with these constructs becoming decreasingly independent as risk increases. Additionally, it is anticipated that variability in the proportion of outcome variance accounted for over a range of risk statuses will be found, with the predictability of child externalizing behavior problems increasing at the extremes on the continuum of risk (i.e., Bernstein & Hans, 1994). Therefore, this study will investigate the efi‘ects of multiple risk and protective factors on children’s development of externalizing behavior problems over time in order to begin to explain and predict the variations in outcome which are observed in children of alcoholics. RISK FACTORS AND CHILD OUTCOME Considerable research has addressed the relationship between risk factors in a child’s environment and adverse child outcomes. Research has found risk factors such as parental psychopathology and marital conflict consistently to be associated with the development of both externalizing andinternalizing child behavior problems, although these relationships are neither unavoidable nor correspond exclusively to specific risk factors and specific child outcomes (Hill & Muka, 1996; Warner, Mufi'son, & Weissman, 1995; Katz & Gottman, 1993; Downey & Walker, 1992; Werner, 1986; Steinhausen, Gobel, & Nestler, 1984). While research has found a variety of factors to be associated with adverse child outcomes, this investigation will focus on those risk factors which have been associated with externalizing behavior problems in children. PM Psychopathology Research has consistently found that children whose parents suffer from a mental illness are at an increased risk for the development of externalizing behavior problems, such as oppositional disorder, conduct disorder, and attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (Jansen et al., 1995; Stanger, Achenbach, & McConaughy, 1993; Hammen, Burge, & Stansbury, 1990; Walker, Downey, & Bergman, 1989). Whereas studies have investigated the impact of a variety of parental psychopathologies on child development, some areas have been more widely researched than others. Parental alcoholism is one risk factor which has been studied extensively (Frick et al., 1992; Jansen et al., 1995; Rubio- 4 Stipec, Bird, Canino, Bravo, & Alegria, 1991; Steinhausen, Gobel, & Nestler, 1984; Werner, 1986; West & Prinz, 1987; Zucker & Fitzgerald, 1991). There is a consensus in the literature that children of alcoholics not only are at a higher risk than the general population for the development of behavior problems but also have a 4 to 5 times greater chance for the development of alcoholism in their lifetime than the children of nonalcoholics (Donovan, 1986; Johnson, Sher, & Rolf, 1991). In a review of the literature investigating the relationship between paternal characteristics and child and adolescent psychopathology, Phares and Compas (1992) report that studies have found a number of childhood problems to be associated with paternal alcoholism, including hyperactivity, conduct disorder, substance abuse, and aggressive behavior. For example, Rubio-Stipec et al. (1991) examined the relationship between parental alcoholism and child outcomes in a community sample of parents and found that the children of alcoholics were significantly more likely to exhibit behavior problems than the ° ren of non-substance abusers. In addition, Jansen et al. (1995) found that those preschool children who exhibited clinically significant levels of behavior problems were more likely to have parents who had a history of alcohol problems and antisocial behavior than those children in the nonclinical range. Children as young as 3 years of age exhibit the adverse efi‘ects of parental alcoholism (Zucker & Fitzgerald, 1991). ucker and Fitzgerald (1991) report that, with -— —.d'". ’m-fi“ M—un—d/ the exception of deprESSiom preschool children between the ages of 3 and 5 who have an alcoholic parent were statistically more impaired in every area of symptomatology measured by the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) than a matched control group of children. In particular, three times as many high-risk children were rated in the clinical 5 range for the CBCL “overall problem behavior” category and the global measures of externalizing symptoms and aggressive behavior (Zucker & Fitzgerald, 1991). The researchers argue that the results of this study, which utilized a sample of preschool children, parallel the results of studies which have found similar relationships between parental alcoholism and behavior problems in older children (e.g., Werner, 1986; Jacob & Leonard, 1986). While an association between paternal alcoholism and child behavior problems has been found repeatedly, the impact of maternal alcoholism has not been as widely investigated. Although theliterature has not thoroughly addressed the relationship between maternal alcoholism and child outcomes, many researchers have suggested that because of the critical role that mothers play in child development, maternal psychopathologies such as alcoholism and depression may significantly impact children (Hill & Muka, 1996; Fitzgerald et al., 1993; Werner, 1986). Fitzgerald et al. (1993) found that while father’s depression, lifetime alcohol problems, and antisocial behavior did not predict child outcomes, the presence of psychopathology in the mother predicted children’s total behavior problems and externalizing behavior problems. In a longitudinal study of the children of alcoholics, Werner (1986) investigated the characteristics of the child and the caregiving environment which differentiated between those children who manifested psychopathology and those who did not. This study found that while equal numbers of children of alcoholic fathers exhibited resiliency or problems, the majority of the children of alcoholic mothers exhibited problem behaviors by the age of 18 (Werner, 1986). 6 In order to address the paucity of maternal alcoholism studies, Hill and Muka (1996) investigated the relationship between maternal alcoholism and children’s psychiatric diagnoses. The researchers utilized a sample of alcoholic women who were receiving treatment for substance abuse and a control sample who were screened for a family history of alcoholism. The results of this study revealed significantly higher rates of psychiatric diagnoses in the children of maternal alcoholics than in the children of controls. [2 . P l D . Research has found that the adverse efi‘ects of parental psychopathology on children are compounded by the presence of multiple parental psychopathologies (Hill & Muka, 1996; Merikangas, Prusofi‘, & Weissman, 1988; Warner et al., 1995). Frick et al. (1991) found higher rates of both substance abuse and antisocial personality disorder in the parents of children who met criteria for conduct disorder or oppositional defiant disorder than in the parents of control children. Studies also have revealed increased rates of psychopathology in the spouses of individuals sufi‘ering fi'om a psychological disorder such as alcoholism (Fitzgerald et al., 1993; Downey & Coyne, 1990; Merikangas et al., 1988; Jacob & Leonard, 1986). For example, Fitzgerald et a1. (1993) found an elevated level of maternal psychopathology, involving alcohol consumption and depression, in the wives of alcoholics, and Merikangas et al. (1988) found significantly higher rates of psychopathology in the spouses of depressed parents compared to controls. Warner et al. (1995) argue that previous research has revealed the importance of examining combinations of risk factors in the prediction of psychiatric disorders. These researchers conducted a study in which they attempted to identify possible mediating and independent variables which were predictive of psychiatric diagnoses in the offspring of 7 parents suffering from a psychiatric disorder (Warner et al., 1995). The results of this study revealed that the risk of Major Depressive Disorder (MDD) in ofi‘spring was significantly increased by the presence of recurrent MDD in the parent (W amer et al., 1995). Additionally, the researchers found that alcohol abuse in coparents significantly increased the risk for disorder in ofi‘spring (Warner et al., 1995). Warner et al. (1995) suggest that a possible explanation for this finding is that risk for psychiatric disorder in offspring is significantly increased as a result of the additive efi‘ects of multiple parental diagnoses. Studies not only have revealed that the spouses of alcoholics have significantly higher rates of psychopathology but also have highlighted the important role which the compounded efl’ect of simultaneous maternal and paternal psychopathology may play in child outcome. Therefore, studies whiCh control for the presence of psychopathology in one parent may not accurately represent the risk mechanisms which impact child development in the general population (F rick et al., 1992; Walker, Downey, & Bergman, 1989). For example, in a study excluding families in which maternal psychopathology was present, Johnson et al. (1989) did not find any significant difi‘erences between the drinking habits or attitudes of the adolescent children of paternal alcoholics, depressives, or controls. The researchers suggest that one possible explanation for the lack of significantly difi‘erent drinking habits may be that not all children of alcoholics are at risk for the development of alcoholism due to the moderating efi‘ects of other child and parent variables (Johnson et al., 1989). However, prior research which has found a significant increase in psychopathology in the Spouses of alcoholics suggests that by eliminating those families in which psychopathology was present in the mother, this sample of children of 8 alcoholics was not representative. Because it may have been biased toward a more functional family environment than is typically found in the families of alcoholics, the sample utilized in this study may have precluded the detection of the possible moderating variables which Johnson et al. (1989) hypothesize play a crucial role in the risk for alcoholism. - Eny_I_r;Q° mm Risk Factors Adverse environmental factors, such as marital conflict and low socioeconomic status, are associated with parental psychopathology as well as with poor child outcomes, such as aggression and conduct problems (Bolger, Patterson, Thompson, & Kupersmidt, 1995; Bond & McMahon, 1984; Corrao, Busellu, Valenti, & Lepore, 1993; Frick et al., 1992; Hill & Muka, 1996; Jacob & Leonard, 1986; Jansen et al., 1995; Chassin, Rogosch, & Barrera, 1992; Rubio-Stipec et al., 1991; Jouriles, Pfimrer, & O’Leary, 1988; Merikangas et al., 1988). Jacob and Leonard (1986) found not only that the fathers and mothers of the most severely impaired children of alcoholics reported more psychological problems, but also that the presence of alcohol-related problems, such as occupational and marital dificulties, were influential in the development of severe child impairment. In a review of the literature concerning the children of depressed parents, Downey and Coyne (1990) report that individuals suffering fiom depression tend to experience interpersonal problems and to marry people who either themselves also sufl‘er fi'om a form of psychopathology or have a family history of psychiatric illness. These researchers also found that the presence of psychiatric illnesses in both spouses is associated with more severe symptomatology, marital disturbance, and family disturbance (Downey & Coyne, 1990). Additionally, Downey and Coyne emphasize that the child problems associated 9 with parental depression are not specific to parental depression and, in fact, the children of depressed parents are similar to other children who experience significant parental illness and the accompanying environmental disturbances. Similarly, Rubio-Stipec et al. (1991) found that children of parents with a psychiatric diagnosis were exposed to a greater number of adverse family environmental factors, such as marital discord and family dysfirnction, than children whose parents did not meet criteria for a disorder. Studies not only have found that environmental variables, such as marital discord, are more common in the families of parents sufi‘ering from a psychopathology, but also have revealed that both environmental factors and parental psychopathology are associated with poor child outcomes (Downey & Coyne, 1990; Rubio-Stipec et al., 1991). Thus, it often remains unclear whether environmental factors afl‘ect child adjustment more directly than simply the presence or absence of a specific parental psychopathology. Chassin, Rogosch, and Barrera (1992) state that while most research has revealed that children of alcoholics exhibit higher rates of externalizing behavior problems, studies have not adequately addressed whether this has resulted fi'om the presence of parental alcoholism or from other factors, such as co-occurring parental psychopathology or disruptions in the family environment. These researchers addressed this issue in a large- scale community and court recruited sample of children of alcoholics and matched community controls and found that while there were unique efi‘ects of environmental stress, parental antisociality, and parental depression, parental alcoholism did not exert unique efi‘ects on child externalizing problems. Research has found marital discord to be associated with the parent-child relationship and poor child outcomes (Black & Pedro-Carroll, 1993; Bond & McMahon, 10 1984; Dadds & Powell, 1991; Katz & Gottman, 1993; Jouriles, Pfimrer, & O’Leary, 1988). Erel and Burman (1995) conducted a meta-analytic review of the literature and found that the quality of the marital relationship is positively related to the quality of the parent-child relationship. Jouriles, Pfifl‘ner, and O’Leary (1988) found both general marital discord and overt marital conflict to be related to maternal reports of toddler conduct problems and waiting-room obserVations of child deviance. Katz and Gottman (1993) found that specific patterns of marital conflict predicted specific child behavior problems 3 years later, with hostile conflict resolution predicting externalizing behavior problems and angry, emotionally distant conflict resolution predicting anxiety and social withdrawal in children (Katz & Gottman, 1993). Similarly, studies also have found low socioeconomic status to be associated with child behavior problems (Bolger et al., 1995; Jansen et al., 1995; Lempers, Clark- Lempers, & Sirnons, 1989). Jansen et al. (1995) found that preschool boys who scored in the clinical range on the “Total Behavior Problems” scale of the CBCL were more likely to have an alcoholic parent and to be of lower socioeconomic status than children who scored in the normal range. Bolger et al. (1995) found that family economic hardship was associated with poor child outcomes, including conduct problems and low self-esteem. In fact, Samerofi‘ and Seifer (1983) have argued that socioeconomic status may have at least as significant an influence on children’s development as other factors that are frequently used to determine risk-status in children, such as parental psychopathology. Multiple Risk Factors Research has found that numerous risk factors are similarly influential on child outcomes and often coexist. The exact nature of the relationship between specific risk 1 1 factors and child outcomes therefore appears to be unclear. Frick et al. (1992) state that the efl‘ects of family risk factors on child development often are confounded, as these variables typically are not independent of each other. Similarly, Walker et al. (1989) argue that the inability to find specific child outcomes associated with specific risk factors has resulted fi'om the fact that risk factors often co-occur and have additive efi'ects. As a result, many researchers have emphasized the importance of investigating the impact of multiple risk factors. on child development (Blackson & Tarter, 1994; Frick et al., 1992; Hill & Muka, 1996; Walker et al., 1989; Samerofi' et al., 1993; Zucker, 1989). Frick et al. (1992) point out that investigating family variables in isolation can not account for the additive or interactional influence of multiple risk factors operating together to influence child outcomes. Blackson and Tarter (1994) also stress the need to consider a “multifactorial model of liability” which“. accounts for individual, family, and peer variables (p. 819). In order to address the apparent paucity of studies examining the impact of multiple, simultaneous risk factors on child development, Walker et al. (1989) investigated both the direct and interactive effects of several risk factors, such as parental psychopathology and child maltreatment, on childhood behavior problems. The researchers report that the findings from their study highlight the need to investigate simultaneously the impact of multiple risk factors, since both main and interactive efl‘ects of parental psychopathology and child maltreatment were found. This study revealed a significant interaction between parental psychopathology and child maltreatment, with children exposed to both maltreatment and parental psychopathology exhibiting increases in externalizing behavior problems over time. The researchers concluded that their 12 investigation of both the direct and interactive efl‘ects of multiple risk factors allowed them to identify more accurately the relationships between risk factors and child behavior problems than the investigation of only main efi‘ects would have allowed (Walker et al., 1989). Researchers have also addressed the impact of multiple risk factors by investigating the relationship between child outcome and the number of environmental risk factors to which a child is exposed. Zucker (1994) argues that investigating the impact of a single risk factor «does not make theoretical sense from a developmental perspective, as it is not the presence of a single risk factor but rather the accumulation of risk over time which is necessary for the development of a disorder. He points out that studies have found more than a 50 percent increase in the probability for a poor outcome when multiple risk factors are present over time (Zucker, 1989). Samerofl‘ et al. (1993) addressed the impact of multiple risk factors in a longitudinal study in which they measured the IQ scores and various social and family risk factors for children at age 4 and then again at age 13 years. By summing the number of high-risk factors each child experienced, such as low socioeconorrric status and maternal psychopathology, the researchers calculated a multiple environmental risk score for each child (Sameroff et al., 1993). Upon examining the relationship between the multiple environmental risk score and IQ at age 4, the researchers found a linear relationship, with those children with no risk factors displaying an average IQ score over 2 standard deviations above those children with the greatest number of risk factors (Sameroff et al., 1993). Furthermore, this study revealed that when the children had reached the age of 13 years, the multiple risk score explained 37% of the variance in child IQ and a multiple l3 regression approach utilizing all 10 risk variables accounted for 50% of the variance (Samerofi‘ et al., 1993). Additionally, Samerofi‘ et al. (1993) determined that the impact of multiple risk factors was independent of both social status and the possible genetic influence of mother’s IQ, as a major efi’ect was still found for the multiple risk score after both SES/race and mother’s IQ had been covaried. Overall, the findings of Sameroff et al. (1993) revealed that it was not the type of risk factor a child encountered that determined outcome, but rather it was the number of risk factors which was influential (Samerofi’ et al., 1993). Sarneroff et al. (1993) state that the results of their study fit a “multicausal model of development,” with similar child outcomes being influenced by various patterns of risk factors rather than a single variable (Samerofi‘ et al., 1993, p. 81). Furthermore, other researchers (e.g., Jessor et al., 1995; Williams, Anderson, McGee, & Silva, 1990) have also found the number of risk factors to be more predictive of childhood psychopathology than the presence of any particular risk factor. Protective Factors and Child Outcome Although this is an area which historically has been neglected in the literature, the importance of investigating factors that protect at-risk individuals has been emphasized recently by many researchers (Sroufe & Rutter, 1984; Rutter, 1987; Garmezy, 1983; Jessor et al., 1995; Grizenko & Fisher, 1992; Rak & Patterson, 1996; Werner, 1986; Garmezy et al., 1984; Wyrnan et al., 1992; Seifer, Samerofl‘, Baldwin, & Baldwin, 1992). Garmezy (1983) states that protective factors are those characteristics that diminish the likelihood of poor outcomes and encourage positive outcomes in individuals who are at risk. Garmezy et al. (1984) argue that the efl‘ects of protective factors, such as IQ, can be conceptualized as “coping” mechanisms that emerge in the presence of stress and reduce the negative impact of the risk factor. When individuals avoid negative outcomes despite having been exposed to stress or risk factors, they are considered to be resilient (Rutter, 1987). Jessor et al. (1995) state that protective factors can have both a direct effect on behavior and moderate the relationship between risk factors and behavior. In this way, a protective factor can decrease the likelihood of a poor outcome independent of the presence of risk factors, as well as interact with risk factors to attenuate the relationship between risk and negative outcomes (Jessor et al. 1995). For example, high child intelligence not only may protect a child fiom negative outcomes but also may decrease the negative impact of the risk factor of paternal alcoholism, thus altering the relationship 14 15 between risk and outcome. While protective factors are hypothesized to afl‘ect outcomes directly as well as to influence the relationship between risk factors and outcomes, Rogosch et al. (1990) argue that the distinction of the mediating or moderating role of protective factors often remains unclear. These researchers assert that mediator variables account for the “how” or “why” of the relationship between a risk factor and an outcome variable, whereas moderator variables address the strength and direction of that relationship (Rogosch et al., 1990). Rogosch et al. (1990) argue that many of the studies which have investigated the role of protective factors in child development have not been designed adequately or have not utilized appropriate statistical analyses, leaving the distinction of the mediating or moderating role of protective factors unclear. In an attempt to correct the limitations of prior research, these researchers investigated the role of protective child personality variables in children of alcoholics and found that child characteristics, such as positive temperament, should be considered moderators rather than mediators of the relationship between risk and outcome (Rogosch et al., 1990). Downey and Walker (1992) argue that interest in those characteristics which may moderate risk often emerged from the observation that despite elevated risk for a poor outcome given a risk factor, not all at-risk children inevitably develop a disorder. For example, studies have found positive outcomes in children who are exposed to risk factors such as parental psychopathology, marital discord, and low socioeconomic status (Werner, 1986; Seifer et al., 1992; Wyman et al., 1992; Warner et al., 1995; Katz & Gottman, 1993). Werner conducted a longitudinal study which followed over 600 children from birth to the age of 32. Approximately one-third of the original sample of children were at 16 high-risk for poor outcomes due to poverty, perinatal stress, and a variety of family risk factors, such as parental psychopathology and marital discord (Werner, 1992). However, Werner (1992) reports that despite having encountered four or more risk factors, one- third of the children classified as high-risk were resilient and developed into healthy, competent adults. Similarly, Wyrnan et al. (1992) found that a significant number of high- risk urban children exhibited resilient outcomes despite having been exposed to a minimum of four and an average of nine stressfirl life events and circumstances, such as parental alcoholism, family discord, and poverty. Researchers have attempted to identify those factors that serve a protective function in the development of at-risk children who are resilient and do not experience poor outcomes, such as behavior problems (Garmezy, 1983). Garmezy (1983) argued that the protective factors identified by Various studies can be organized into three general categories of factors: positive child characteristics, positive family environment, and external support systems. Subsequently, this has been supported by numerous researchers (Werner, 1986; Werner, 1990; Grizenko & Fisher, 1992; Garmezy, et al., 1984; Rak & Patterson, 1996; Wyrnan et al., 1992; Chase-Landsdale, Wakschlag, & Brooks-Gum, 1995; Smith & Prior, 1995). The following discussion of protective factors will be organized into two sections, child characteristics and family environment, based on the categories set forth by Garmezy (1983). External support systems, although important to the development of children, will not be discussed in this review, as these protective factors typically are not available to children until they have reached school-age and have access to sources of support independent of their immediate family. 17 Research consistently has identified several personal attributes as characteristic of resilient children (Garmezy, 1983; Garmezy et al., 1984; Werner, 1986; Rogosch, Chassin, & Sher, 1990; Rutter, 1987;1(yrios & Prior, 1991; Herrenkohl, Herrenkohl, & Egolf, 1994; Fergusson & Lynskey, 1996;.Masten et al., 1988; Smith & Prior, 1995; Wyman et al., 1991; Tschann, et al., 1996; Seifer et al., 1992; Varni, Rubenfeld, Talbot, & Setoguchi, 1990; Corbo-Richert, 1994). Two such attributes to be discussed in this review due to their relevance to the development of externalizing behavior problems in children are high intellectual ability and positive temperament characteristics. Studies have found these factors to play a protective role in the development of children exposed to a variety of risk factors, ranging from parental alcoholism (Werner, 1986; Rogosh et al., 1990) to physical disorders (Lavigrre & Faier-Routman, 1993; Vami et al., 1990; Corbo- Richert, 1994). Intelligmg. Research has found high intellectual ability (high IQ) to be associated with better outcomes as well as to be protective in the development of children exposed to risk (Garmezy et al., 1984; Masten et al., 1988; Herrenkohl et al., 1994; Fergusson & Lynskey, 1996). In a study using a community sample of children, Masten et al. (1988) investigated the moderating effect of hypothesized protective factors on the relationship between stress and children’s competence in school. The researchers utilized several competency dimensions, including classroom disruptiveness, engagement in school activities, and academic achievement, and found that the relationship between stress and children’s competence was dependent upon individual characteristics as well as the competence 18 criterion. The results of this study revealed that high IQ protected against the adverse impact of stress on the competency variable of classroom disruptiveness whereas low IQ protected against the adverse impact of stress when the competency criterion of classroom engagement was utilized (Masten et al., 1988). The researchers hypothesize that while a high IQ protects children against the adverse effects of stress on classroom disruptiveness, the effects of stress are apparent in the lives of high IQ children when the realm of classroom engagement is considered, with the opposite outcome being found for low IQ children (Masten et al., 1988). Other researchers have investigated the protective function of high intellectual ability in at-risk children’s development of emotional problems. Fergusson and Lynskey (1996) conducted a sixteen-year longitudinal study of a birth cohort of 940 children in an attempt to identify those factors which were characteristic of children who had been exposed to high levels of family adversity, such as marital conflict and low SES, and did not experience any externalizing behavior problems, such as substance use, conduct disorder, and juvenile delinquency. The researchers found that resiliency in children was associated with a higher IQ. Furthermore, similar to other studies addressing the impact of multiple risk factors (i.e. Samerofl‘ et al., 1993), Fergusson and Lynskey (1996) addressed the impact of multiple protective factors by investigating the relationship between child outcome and the combination of protective factors present in a child’s development. The researchers report that while individual protective factors were only modestly associated with resilient outcomes, a combined resiliency factor, consisting of measures of family adversity, IQ at age eight, novelty seeking, and afliliations with delinquent peers, was strongly predictive of resilience. This finding is consistent with 19 those of Jessor et al. (1995), who found a summative index of protective factors to moderate the relationship between risk and poor outcomes. Although numerous studies have found high intellectual ability to serve a protective firnction for children exposed to risk, Downey and Walker (1992) report that the protective function of child characteristics needs to be considered in conjunction with other variables. In a study which investigated the role of both child and family characteristics in at-risk children’s development of aggression and depression, these researchers found that IQ was not associated with outcomes when family-level influences, such as parental psychopathology and child maltreatment, were controlled. Temperament. Studies have also found positive temperament to be associated with better outcomes in children (Fergusson & Lynskey, 1996; Werner, 1986; Varni et al., 1990; Corbo-Richert, 1994; Smith & Prior, 1995; Wyman et al., 1991; Tschann et al., 1996; Kyrios & Prior, 1991). Characteristics of positive temperament include flexibility, social responsiveness, autonomy, and positive afi‘ect. Wyman et al. (1991) investigated those factors which were protective in the lives of high-risk urban children who had been exposed to a minimum of four and an average of nine stressfirl life events and circumstances, such as parental alcoholism, family discord, and poverty. The researchers classified children as stress-resilient at ages 10-12 based on high scores on both parent and teacher ratings of adjustment in domains such as classroom behavior and peer relationships. The results of this study revealed that easy temperament ratings in infancy difl‘erentiated between stress-resistant and stress-afiected children (Wyman et al., 1991). Similarly, Werner (1986) found those children of alcoholics who did not develop a coping 20 problem such as delinquency to be characterized by easy temperament and average to above average intelligence. Smith and Prior (1995) applied an intra-family‘ design to determine those characteristics of school-age children which protected them from negative outcomes despite high levels of familial psychosocial stress, such as financial problems and drug addiction. ‘The researchers classified children as resilient if all scores were in the non- clinical range on both the CBCL and the Teacher’s Report Form in the areas of social competence, home and school behavioral adjustment, and adaptive functioning (Smith & Prior, 1995). Smith and Prior (1995) found that teachers tended to rate children more positively than mothers. These researchers report that teacher ratings of a temperament factor, consisting of characteristics such as easiness and likability, discriminated more accurately between those children who were classified as resilient and those who experienced behavior problems than any other variable investigated in this study, including maternal warmth and the number of negative life events experienced by the child. While many of the studies investigating the role of protective factors in children’s outcomes have historically utilized school-age children, researchers have begun to examine the protective impact of child characteristics on outcomes in preschool children (Kyrios & Prior, 1991; Tschann et al., 1996). Tschann et al. (1996) found that pre-school children with difiicult temperaments experienced more externalizing and internalizing behavior problems overall than children with easy temperaments. They also report that the efl‘ects of family conflict on preschoolers behavior problems were moderated by an easy temperament (Tschann et al., 1996). The researchers hypothesize that an easy temperament may protect children in high-conflict environments by decreasing the 21 likelihood that they will be the target of negative responses from family members. Similarly, Kyrios and Prior (1991) found that positive temperament characteristics decreased the adverse effects of family risk factors, such as marital problems and parental psychopathology, on preschool children’s behavioral adjustment, such as aggressiveness and overactivity. A potential problem with this study is the degree of overlap which exists between the measures of temperament and child behavioral adjustment, each of which assess many of the same characteristics, such as manageability and activity level. MW Considerable research has also found characteristics of the family environment to be associated with positive outcomes in children who are exposed to risk factors (Smith & Prior, 1995; Seifer et al., 1992; Pettit & Bates, 1989; Grossman et al., 1992; Rubenstein, Heeren, Housman, Rubin, & Stechler, 1989; Weist, Freedman, Paskewitz, Proescher, & Flaherty, 1995; Bradley et al., 1995; Bradley, Caldwell, & Rock, 1988; Bradley & Caldwell, 1981). Characteristics of the family environment which will be discussed in this review are family cohesion, maternal warmth, and a stimulating physical and psychological home environment, including factors such as cleanliness and availability of books. Similar to the results found regarding protective child characteristics, studies have found family environment factors to play a protective role in the development of children exposed to a variety of risk factors, ranging from parental alcoholism (Zucker & Gomberg, 1986) to pediatric medical problems (Phipps & Mulhem, 1995; Varni et al., 1990). Fm]! Cohesion, Research has found family cohesion to be associated with better outcomes and to be protective in the development of children exposed to risk (Phipps & Mulhern, 1995; 22 Seifer et al., 1992; Pettit & Bates, 1989; Grossman et al., 1992; Rubenstein, Heeren, Housman, Rubin, & Stechler, 1989; Weist, Freedman, Paskewitz, Proescher, & Flaherty, 1995). Family cohesion is a concept that addresses a family’s emotional closeness, mutual supportiveness, and involvement in activities together. While a family environment characterized by cohesion may be more likely to be characterized by the absence of family risk factors such as family conflict, it is theoretically possible for a family to be cohesive and to engage in conflictual interactions. In fact, Moos and Moos (1986) report that while family cohesion and family conflict subscales of a family environment scale are modestly inversely correlated (-.46), these subscales measure distinct, although related, aspects of the family environment. Phipps and Mulhern (1995) investigated the relationship between aspects of the family environment and resilience to the stress of bone marrow transplant in children. These researchers report that-pediatric bone marrow transplant provides an excellent opportunity for the study of resiliency in children because it is comprised of several stressful components, including the isolation from fiiends and family which often accompanies the procedure, the life-threatening nature of the procedure, and the duration and intensity of the procedure (Phipps & Mulhern, 1995). Phipps and Mulhern (1995) measured child adjustment in the areas of behavior problems, social competence, and self- esteem both prior to hospital admission and 6-12 months after the procedure was completed. The results of this study revealed that while family cohesion was not related to measures of adjustment prior to hospitalization, it served as a protective factor against the stress of bone marrow transplant and was predictive of children’s adjustment at follow-up. Research has also found that the role of family cohesion as a protective factor may vary by the gender of the child (Weist et al., 1995; Grossman et al., 1992). Weist et al. 23 ( 1995) attempted to identify factors that played a protective role in the behavioral adjustment and school performance of at-risk urban children. The results of this study revealed that family cohesion was the only variable that protected boys exposed to stressful life events against adverse consequences, such as externalizing behavior problems. Family cohesion was not found to play a protective role in the lives of girls; instead, it was associated with dificulties at school. The researchers hypothesized that family cohesion did not appear to play a protective role in the lives of girls because girls may be more likely to focus on social relationships during times of stress, and, therefore, may encounter dificulties at school as their attention is diverted away from schoolwork and towards relationships with family members and peers. Because the results of this study were based on a cross-sectional sample, it is important to keep in mind that inferences can not be made regarding the causal relationships among variables. Grossman et al. (1992) also investigated the relationship between family variables and positive outcomes in at-risk children. These researchers report that identified protective factors, such as family cohesion and good communication with parents, did not interact with and mitigate the negative efl‘ects of risk factors, but rather, had a direct impact on outcome independent of risk (Grossman et al., 1992). These researchers found that family cohesion protected girls against delinquency, poor grades, distressed mood, and low self-esteem. However, the results of this study revealed that family cohesion was only protective against delinquency and low self-esteem for boys. W Studies have also found that a stimulating physical and psychological home environment, including characteristics such as cleanliness, adequate living space, and the 24 provision of toys and reading materials, is associated with better outcomes in children (Bradley et al., 1995; Bradley, Caldwell, & Rock, 1988; Bradley, Caldwell, Rock, & Harris, 1986). Although ofien inversely related to SES, the quality of the home environment has been found to be influential in children’s development independent of SES (Bradley and Caldwell, 1984). Bradley et al. (1994) found that the home environments of resilient children living in poverty who did not exhibit behavior problems or developmental delays were more stimulating, safer, and less crowded than those of comparably at—risk children who were not functioning adequately in these areas. Bradley et al. (1988) conducted a study in which they assessed the home environments of children when they were infants and then reassessed these characteristics of the home when the children were 2 years old and when they were between 10 and 11 years of age. These researchers then investigated the relationship between aspects of the home environment over time and children’s classroom behavior in areas such as hostility and distractibility at the age of 10 or 11. They found that having a variety of developmentally stimulating materials in the home environment at all three points in time was significantly related to children’s classroom behavior at the age of 10. Furthermore, the researchers found that the organization of the physical environment at age 2 was related to children’s classroom behavior at age 10. Bradley et al. (1995) also investigated the relationship between aspects of the home environment of low birth weight children and adaptive social behavior, such as disruptiveness and compliance, over time. These researchers found that the quality and the quantity of stimulation available to the child in the home environment, including a variety of developmentally stimulating experiences and learning materials, was significantly 25 predictive of adaptive social behavior in this at-risk group of children (Bradley et al., 1995). Furthermore, Bradley et al. (1995) also reported that aspects of the home environment appeared to be predictive of adaptive behavior independent of SES or family structure. Mategrfl Warmth. Researchers have found another aspect of the family environment, maternal warmth, to be associated with better outcomes and to be protective in the development of children exposed to risk (Smith & Prior, 1995; Bradley et al., 1991; Pettit & Bates, 1989; Bradley & Caldwell, 1981). Maternal warmth is a concept that addresses mother’s positive afl’ect directed at the child, such as pride, afi‘ection, and positive physical contact. In a study which utilized naturalistic observation methods to assess family interaction patterns, Pettit and Bates (1989) investigated the relationship between aspects of the family relationship and child behavior problems over time. These researchers derived a composite variable, mother positivity, fi'om measures of maternal affectionate contact, afl‘ectionate teaching, and verbal stimulation. The results of this study revealed that mother positivity assessed over a child’s first two years of life was protective against behavior problems at the age of four (Pettit & Bates, 1989). Furthermore, Pettit and Bates (1989) report that mother positivity early in life is predictive of future behavioral outcomes independent of age for measures of maternal positivity. However, the results of this study should be interpreted with caution, as an assessment of child behavior problems independent of potentially confounding maternal variables was not obtained. A study which had been discussed in this review in relation to the protective function of positive temperament also found maternal warmth to be associated with 26 positive child outcomes (Smith & Prior, 1995). Smith and Prior (1995) utilized an intra- farnily design to detemrine those characteristics of school-age children exposed to psychosocial stressors which protected them from negative outcomes. These researchers not only discovered that a positive temperament served a protective role in the lives of the resilient at-risk children, but they also found that mother-child warmth, which assessed mother’s verbal and emotional responsivity to the child, was associated with child competence both at home and at school (Smith & Prior, 1995). The researchers argue that the importance of a warm mother-child relationship is emphasized by the cross- situational protection it provided in the lives of the resilient children in this study. Relationship Between Risk and Protective Factors Orthogonality of Constructs Many researchers argue that protective factors, although often negatively correlated with risk factors, are not merely at the opposite end of a continuum of risk from risk factors, but are separate concepts which cart, and often do, coexist (Jessor et al., 1995; Rutter, 1987). Jessor et al. (1995) state that while empirical studies often find high risk environments lacking in protective factors, it is possible for high protection to occur with high risk. To illustrate this point, the researchers suggest that it is logically possible for an adolescent to have delinquent friendships while maintaining high academic involvement and achievement (Jessor et al., 1995). Jessor et al. (1995) argue that risk and protective factors should therefore be conceptualized as orthogonal variables, despite the fact that they are often experienced as inverse concepts. Tabachnick and Fidel] (1996) define orthogonality as a “perfect nonassociation between variables,” with knowledge of the value of one factor providing no information regarding the value of another factor (p. 9). In order to provide empirical support for the conceptual argument that risk and protection should be treated as discrete categories, Jessor et al. (1995) investigated the relationship between risk and protective factors for adolescent problem behavior in a sample recruited from the 7th, 8th, and 9th grades of a large metropolitan school district. 27 28 The researchers report that the initial participation rate for their study was not optimal, as only 67% of the middle school students and 49% of the high school students participated (Jessor et al., 1995). Jessor et al. (1995) utilized several statistical methods to explore the relationship between risk and protective variables and to provide support for their hypothesis that these factors should be treated as orthogonal variables rather than as inverse correlates. First, the researchers investigated the proportion of variance shared by a summative index of risk and a summative index of protection and found that these indexes shared only a modest proportion (18%) of common variance. The researchers then investigated the degree to which individual risk and protective factors which seemed to be on opposite ends of a continuum, such as exposure to fiiends who model problem behavior and exposure to fiiends who model conventional behavior, were correlated and found only a modest relationship (r = -.20). Additionally, Jessor et al. (1995) found that these potentially nonorthogonal individual risk and protective factors retained significant beta weights at the final step of hierarchical multiple regressions for outcome variables such as delinquent behavior and problem drinking. Furthermore, Jessor et al. (1995) found that the degree to which risk and protective variables correlated with other measures, including both independent and outcome variables, varied. The researchers conclude that high risk does not necessarily imply low protection and, therefore, a distinction between protection and risk remains a valuable objective. However, Jessor et al. (1995) also report that the generalizability of their findings may be limited by difiiculties with their sample, such as a low initial participation rate and a high attrition rate. The researchers state that while conceptually a distinction between risk and protection appears meaningful and empirically their study provides preliminary 29 support for this difl‘erentiation, firrther research utilizing samples which include extreme scores on both risk and protective factors would provide additional information regarding the relationship between these constructs (Jessor et al., 1995). Implications of Sm]; Vm’abilig While Jessor et al.'s (1995) study provides support for the coexistence of risk and protection in some populations, the issues raised by the researchers regarding problems with their sample and consequently the generalizability of their results should not be dismissed. Sher and Trull (1996) emphasize that although many researchers often find it necessary to utilize convenience samples, the population from which a sample is chosen has many important implications for the research findings. These researchers caution that nonprobability sampling not only is often subject to selection bias but also jeopardizes the external validity of research findings (site: & Trull, 1996). It seems that adolescents engaging in problem behaviors such as delinquency and substance use, the outcome variables utilized by Jessor et al. (1995), may not be accurately represented in a volunteer sample of students. Additionally, Kazdin and Kagan (1994) argue that risk factors often do not exist in isolation but rather are interrelated or appear in “packages” (p. 39). For example, these researchers state that the presence of a risk factor for child psychopathology, such as low SES, increases the likelihood of the presence of other child risk factors, such as poor parenting, as these factors are frequently positively related. As those adolescents characterized by extremely high risk scores may not have volunteered or obtained parental consent to participate in Jessor et al.'s (1995) study, the generalizability of these findings to high-risk samples is potentially limited. 30 Kazdin and Kagan (1994) state that developmental psychopathology research typically investigates main effects and linear relationships between variables such as risk factors and outcomes. However, the researchers argue that these assumptions are often violated, as many nonlinear relationships and interaction effects have been identified in studies of development and psychopathology. They therefore suggest that the impact of multiple variables on outcome be conceptualized nonlinearly in order to more accurately capture the relationship among variables. However, McClelland and Judd (1993) state that there often exist significant statistical dimculties in detecting interactions and moderator efi‘ects in field studies which often do not exist in experiments. The researchers argue that often the distributions of independent variables in field studies are more restricted, resulting in lower residual variance of the interaction term. In this way, it can be seen that restrictions in the extreme'ranges of variables such as risk factors in field studies can produce results which do not accurately reflect relationships between variables, such as moderator effects. While the possibility of difl‘erential relationships between risk and protection over a range of risk severity could not be determined in the sample utilized in the Jessor et al. (1995) study, a study conducted by Bernstein and Hans (1994) illustrates the potential implications of a restricted range of risk on a study’s findings. Bernstein and Hans (1994) found that for a high-risk group, determined by prenatal exposure to methadone, a cumulative risk variable predicted four developmental child outcomes, such as child attention and cooperativeness, although this variable did not predict child outcome in the low-risk comparison group. This study revealed consistently higher correlations between the high-risk group’s cumulative risk index and child outcome measures than for the low- 3 1 risk group or the entire sample as a whole (Bernstein & Hans, 1994). In discussing this finding, the researchers argue that the higher correlations for the high-risk group result fi'om this group’s “greater range of risk factors,” as there existed greater outcome predictability for those children on the extreme ends of risk, with those children categorized as high-risk who had fewer risk factors than the comparison group exhibiting predictable resilient outcomes while those who had more risk factors than any comparison child consistently displaying very poor outcomes at follow-up (Bernstein & Hans, 1994, p. 355) In a study utilizing a high risk sample of children of alcoholics and a control sample, Moses (1992) investigated the relationship between paternal alcohol problems and child behavior problems, as well as the moderating efi‘ects of contextual risk variables, such as parental antisocial behavior and. depression, and contextual protective factors, such as SES and parental intelligence. This study found that while these contextual risk and protective variables all demonstrated significant main efl‘ects on child behavior problems, contextual protective factors did not moderate the relationship between paternal . alcohol problems and child outcome. However, this study did not include many of the protective factors identified by previous research (i.e. Garmezy, 1983), such as child characteristics or aspects of the family environment other than SES or parental IQ. A significant interaction was found between parental psychopathology and paternal alcohol problems, with alcohol problems demonstrating greater predictability of child outcome when accompanied by low parent psychopathology. Moses (1992) argues that this finding of a decreased predictability of child behavior problems from paternal alcohol problems as parental psychopathology increases may result from the increased levels of 32 family problems attributable to the presence of parental psychopathology. In this way, the impact on the family environment and child outcome which the presence of paternal alcohol problems can contribute independently is attenuated by the effect of additional psychopathology on these same variables. Although Moses (1992) found statistically significant, low-order correlations between risk and protective factors in this high-risk population, this study did not investigate the relationship between these factors over varying levels of parental psychopathology. Studies utilizing subjects representing extreme risk or extreme protection may reveal that there exists a differential relationship between risk and protection over the continuum of risk, with these constructs beconring decreasingly independent as risk increases. Additionally, it may be that outcomes are more predictable ficm measures of risk and protection in those subpopulations on the extremes of the continuum of risk than for those subjects in the middle range. Hypotheses The literature review above indicates that a variety of factors may place children at risk for externalizing behavior problems, increase the likelihood of positive outcomes, or diminish the likelihood of externalizing behavior problems in individuals who are at risk. Whereas researchers (e.g., Samerofi’ et al., 1993; Jessor et al., 1995) have found that the amount of risk and protection to which children are exposed is significantly related to outcomes, prior research typically has not investigated the relationship between risk and protection or examined difi'erences in outcome predictability over a range of risk statuses. Additionally, research generally has not adequately addressed the roles of mediating and moderating variables. As a result, an understanding of the relationship between risk and protective factors and children’s outcomes remains unclear. The current investigation will examine the relationship between risk and protective variables, potential mediating and moderating efl’ects of these variables on child externalizing behavior problems, and variability in the proportion of outcome variance accounted for over a range of risk statuses in a longitudinal, community-based sample of male children of alcoholics and demographically-matched male children of non-alcoholics. Several characteristics of the family and the environment when the targeted children are between the ages of three and five (time 1), which the literature suggests are associated with parental alcoholism and poor child outcomes, are expected to increase the likelihood of child externalizing behavior problems three years later when the children are between 33 34 the ages of six and eight (time 2) and will be referred to as risk factors. Additionally, several characteristics of the child at time 1 which have been associated with resiliency in children are expected to diminish the likelihood of child externalizing behavior problems at time 2 independent of the presence of risk factors as well as to moderate the relationship between risk and negative outcomes. Similarly, characteristics of the family environment at time 1 which have been associated with resiliency in children are expected to be associated with more positive child outcomes and to account for the effects of risk on child behavior problems. These characteristics of the child and the family environment which are expected to be associated with lower levels of child externalizing behavior problems will be referred to as protective factors. A cumulative risk index will be calculated by summing the number of risk factors impacting each child, as determined by criteria specified for each risk variable in this study’s description of measures. A model of the hypothesized relationships among variables is provided in Figure 1. Several specific hypotheses have been developed and are described below. Predicted Main Efi‘ects 1) Numerous studies have found paternal psychopathology, maternal psychopathology, low SES, and marital conflict to place a child at risk for negative outcomes (e.g., Hill & Muka, 1996; Jansen et al., 1995; Werner, 1986). The current investigation expects to find positive main effects between risk factors at time 1 and externalizing behavior problems at time 2. 2) It is expected that risk factors will be negatively related to family environment protective factors, with the strength of this relationship increasing with increased levels of risk. For example, SES is expected to be negatively related to a stimulating home 35 environment, although it is anticipated that the strength of the relationship between these variables will decrease with the level of risk. 3) Numerous studies have found high child IQ and positive child temperament to be associated with positive outcomes (e. g., Garmezy et al., 1984; Fergusson & Lynskey, 1996; Smith & Prior, 1995; Werner, 1986). The current investigation expects to find that child protective factors at time 1 have a direct inverse effect on child externalizing behavior problems at time 2. 4) Previous research has found family cohesion, a stimulating home environment, and maternal warmth to be associated with positive outcomes (e.g., Phipps & Mulhern, 1995; Bradley et al., 1995; Smith & Prior, 1995). The current investigation expects to find that family environment protective factors at time 1 are negatively related to child externalizing behavior problems at tirneg2. Predictg Modgatm’ g Efl‘ects 5) Researchers have found that protective child characteristics, such as positive temperament, moderate rather than mediate the relationship between risk and outcome (e.g., Rogosch et al., 1990). This investigation hypothesizes that the statistical interaction of time 1 child protective factors with risk factors will moderate the main efi‘ects of risk on child externalizing behavior problems at time 2. The relationship between risk factors and child externalizing behavior problems is expected to be greater for children low on a child protective characteristic than for children high on a child protective characteristic. Prflictfl Mediating Efi‘ects 6) As outlined above, family environment protective factors are hypothesized to be inversely related to child externalizing behavior problems, and risk factors are expected 36 to predict both child externalizing behavior problems and family environment protective factors. It is anticipated that the strength of the relationship between risk factors and child externalizing behavior problems will be significantly weakened when the efi’ects of family environment protective factors are considered. Therefore, time 1 protective family environment factors are hypothesized to mediate the main effect of risk factors on child externalizing behavior problems at time 2 (Baron & Kenny, 1986; Rogosch et al., 1990). Predicted Effects of Risk Level 7) It is hypothesized that the strength of the relationship between child externalizing behavior problems and the proposed risk variables will difi‘er over the continuum of risk, with the predictability of child externalizing behavior problems increasing at the extremes on the continuum of risk. A graph of the hypothesized predictability of child externalizing behavior problems over the continuum of risk is provided in Figure 2. 8) The current investigation expects to find differential relationships between risk and both child and family environment protection along the continuum of risk, with these constructs becoming decreasingly independent as risk increases. More specifically, subpopulations at the high end of risk are expected to have greater inverse correlations of risk and protection than subpopulations at the low end of risk. A graph of the hypothesized relationship between risk and protection over the continuum of risk is provided in Figure 3. Methods $1M Subjects in the current investigation are 220 families participating in the Michigan State University-University of Michigan Longitudinal Study (MSU-UM; Fitzgerald et al., 1993; Zucker & Fitzgerald, 1991). This ongoing study is comprised Of two population- based samples of alcoholic families and a contrast sample of non-alcoholic families, all of whom had a biological son between the ages of 3 and 6 at the beginning of this study. The samme of children utilized in this study is comprised entirely of Caucasian males. As the original focus of this longitudinal project was on the development of alcohol problems, behavior problems, and related symptomatology in a high risk sample of children of alcoholics, this study concentrated on males due to their increased risk for the development of these dificulties (Zucker, Fitzgerald, & Moses, 1995). Given literature supporting the relationship between patterns of alcohol involvement and ethnic/racial status, the limited ethnic/racial composition of this sample was determined by this study’s locale and the resulting inability to efl’ectively consider this covariate, as census data revealed that less than 10% of the population in the area where data collection took place consisted of other ethnic and racial groups. All recruited families had been invited to participate in a long term study of family health and child development. The first group of alcoholic families was recruited from the population of all convicted male drunk drivers in four counties in the Mid-Michigan area having a blood 37 38 alcohol concentration of at least 0.15% or a blood alcohol concentration ofO. 12% with a history of prior alcohol-related arrests. Additional inclusionary criteria for this study were: the men were living with a biological son between 3 and 5 years of age (the target child); the men were living with the target child’s mother; and the men met Feighner et al. (1972) criteria for a probable or definite diagnosis of alcoholism both during recruitment, based on the Short Michigan Alcohol Screening Test (SMAST, Selzer, 1975), and during data collection, based on the Diagnostic Interview Schedule, Version 111 (DIS; Robins, Helzer, Crougharr, & Ratclifi‘e, 1980). The second group of alcoholics were recruited during efforts to identify a community contrast sample for this study. Door-to-door community canvassing of the neighborhoods in which the court-recruited alcoholics lived was used to recruit contrast families within the same census tract who also had two parents and a biological son between the ages of 3 and 6 residing within the home (Fitzgerald et al., 1995). Those families identified through the door-to-door canvassing who met study criteria and had a father who met criteria for alcoholism but did not have a history of drunk driving convictions were placed in a second group of alcoholic families, the community alcoholic group. Inclusion in the contrast sample was contingent upon neither of the parents meeting F eighner criteria for alcoholism or other drug abuse/dependence. Although mother’s alcoholism status was assessed for all groups, it was neither a requirement or a basis for exclusion except for in the control sample, in which neither parent could meet criteria for a substance problem. As the original sample was recruited from the population of all convicted male drunk drivers in four counties and the contrast samples were recruited fi'om the neighborhoods of the court-recruited alcoholics, this sample is 39 comprised of families who represent various points along continuums of both individual risk factors, such as SES, as well as overall risk. Mars The current investigation utilizes two waves of data fi'om the MSU-UM Longitudinal Study. The first wave of data was collected fi'om the families when the biological sons targeted for this study were between the ages of 3 and 5 years. The second wave of data was obtained approximately three years later when the targeted children were between 6 and 8 years of age. Each family participating in this study completes numerous questionnaires, direct observation sessions, and interviews during each wave of data collection. The data are collected by trained project stafl‘ who are blind to family risk status over the course of nine contacts, involving approximately 15 hOurs of time. Seven of the data collection contacts occur in the family home, and two occur on a university campus. F arnilies receive financial compensation for their participation in the longitudinal project. eres The current investigation utilizes a subset of the battery of questionnaires collected during the first two waves of the MSU-UM Longitudinal Study. Risk factors examined were assessed at time 1 and include the following: paternal psychopathology, as measured by alcoholism, depression, and antisociality; maternal psychopathology, as measured alcoholism, depression, and antisociality; low socioeconomic status; and marital conflict. Protective factors examined were collected during time 1 and include positive child characteristics, as measured by high child intelligence and positive child temperament, and positive family environment characteristics, as measured by family cohesion, maternal 40 warmth, and stimulation provided by the physical and psychologic a1 home environment. The dependent variable, child externalizing behavior problems, was measured at time 2. The specific measures used in the current investigation are described below. Additionally, a cumulative index of risk was created for each subject by counting the total number of risk factors which were present (Samerofi‘ et al., 1987; Bernstein & Hans, 1994). Each risk variable was dichotomized according to criteria specified below in order to determine the presence or absence of that risk factor for each subject. As eight risk factors were investigated in this study, the cumulative risk index ranged from zero to eight. (For example, if one subject’s maternal depression, paternal alcoholism, and family conflict scores were all above the cut-off values outlined in the discussion of measures below and the levels of the other risk factors were below their respective cut-offs, the cumulative risk index for this subject would equal three). Similarly, a cumulative index of protection was created for each subject by counting the total number of protective factors which were present for each subject based upon the cut-offs for each protective variable outlined below (F ergusson & Lynskey, 1996). As five protective factors were investigated in this study, the cumulative protection index ranged from zero to five. Measures of Risk Short Michigan Alcohol Screening Test (SMAST, Selzer, 1975). As outlined in the discussion of subjects, the SMAST is used to classify fathers and mothers as alcoholic or nonalcoholic prior to their participation in this study based upon Feighner et al. (1972) criteria for a probable or definite diagnosis of alcoholism. Questions such as “Do you feel you are a normal drinker” and “Are you able to stop drinking when you want to” are answered in a yes/no format. This instrument is a shortened l3-item version of the 41 Michigan Alcoholism Screening Test (MAST) and has been found to be a reliable and valid screening test for alcoholism (Selzer, 1975). A reliability coeficient alpha of .93 has been reported, and studies have found scores on this measure to discriminate between groups of alcoholics and nonalcoholics (Selzer, 197 5). Disgpestie Intfl'ew Schflple, Versien III (DIS) (Robins, Helzer, Croughan, & Ratclifi‘e, 1980). As outlined in the discussion of subjects, father’s and mother’s responses on the DIS are used to verify the initial classification of alcoholic or nonalcoholic based upon Feighner et al. (1972) criteria fora probable or definite diagnosis of alcoholism. This instrument is a highly structured interview which is designed to assess symptomatology in a range of areas and to make diagnoses based upon DSM-IIIR, Feighner criteria, and Research Diagnostic Criteria. The DIS has been found to have adequate reliability and validity, with a reported mean kappa of .69, a mean sensitivity of 75%, and a mean specificity of 94 % across all DSM-IIIR diagnoses (Robins et al., 1980). Paternal and/or maternal alcoholism were included in the cumulative risk index based upon each parent’s classification as alcoholic or nonalcoholic. Beek Depression Invmteg, Short Form (B_DI) (Beck, Steer, & Garbin, 1988). This self-report questionnaire consists of 13 items that ask respondents to indicate the degree to which they are currently experiencing depressive symptomatology, such as crying easily, appetite disturbances, and sleep disturbances. The BDI has been found to be a valid and reliable test for depression, with a reported mean coeficient alpha of .81 for nonpsychiatric samples (Beck, Steer, & Garbin, 1988). Studies have found scores on the short form of this instrument to correlate between .89 and .97 with the long form (Beck, Steer, Garbin, 1988). Paternal and maternal depression were included in the cumulative 42 risk index based upon a score of four or greater, as this cut-ofi‘ has been established for this instrument as a classification of mild to more severe depression. Antiseg'fl Behavier Chgklig (Zucker & Noll, 1980). This self-report questionnaire consists of 46 items that ask respondents to indicate the fi'equency with which they have participated in delinquent, criminal, and antisocial activities throughout their lifetime. This instrument has been found to have adequate test-retest reliability (.91 over four weeks) and internal consistency (reliability coeficient alpha = .93) in a series of studies with populations ranging fiom male and female college students to male and female inmates (Zucker & Noll, 1980; Zucker, Noll, Ham, Fitzgerald, & Sullivan, 1994). Additionally, these studies have found this instrument to differentiate between individuals with long histories of antisocial behavior (prisoners), individuals with minor ofl’enses in district court, and university students (Zucker et al., 1994). Utilizing previously established criteria for this instrument, paternal antisociality was included in the cumulative risk index based upon a cut-ofi‘ score of 24, and maternal antisociality was included based upon a cut-off score of 18 (Bingham, Zucker, & Fitzgerald, 1996; Caplan, 1996). Revised Duncan Seciogonomic [neg (TSE12) (Stevens & Featherman, 1981; Mueller & Parcel, 1981). This instrument is an index of occupational attainment and was used to measure father’s and mother’s Socioeconomic Status (SES), with family SES being defined in the current study as the average of father’s and mother’s TSE12 scores or the principle earner’s SES when there exists only one principle earner in the family (Hauser, 1994). Based upon research which has indicated that occupation-based measures of SES are the most reliable and valid single measures of SES, the TSE12 was utilized in 43 this study to determine family SES rather than a measure of income (Mueller & Parcel, 1981). SES was included in the cumulative risk index based upon a score below the median split of the distribution of this variable in this sample (Bingham, Zucker, & Fitzgerald, 1996). Bingharn et a1. (1996) argue that the use of the median as a breakpoint for the determination of risk versus lack of risk produces a measure of cumulative risk which more accurately represents the total range of risk experienced by the sample. Additionally, the researchers state that this procedure is less likely than extreme groups approaches to result in heteroscadastic group variances or to truncate variance (Bingharn et al., 1996). Fm’ly Envirenmmt Selle (ES) (Moos & Moos, 1986). The FES is a self-report questionnaire that consists of 90 true-false items designed to measure the social- environmental attributes of the family. This instrument has ten subscales which assess the family on three broader dimensions: relationship, personal growth, and system maintenance. The FES subscale, conflict, from the relationship dimension of this instrument measures the amount of anger, aggression, and conflict openly expressed - among family members. Items from the conflict subscale include “We fight alot in our family” and “Family members sometimes hit each other.” Family conflict is defined in the current study as mother’s FES conflict subscale score. This measure has been found to have acceptable test-retest reliability, ranging from .68 to .86 for the various subscales, as well as adequate internal consistency (Moos & Moos, 1986). Additionally, the FES has been found to successfully discriminate between distressed and normal families (Moos & Moos, 1986). Conflict was included in the cumulative risk index based upon a score above the median split of the distribution of this variable in this sample (Bingham, Zucker, & Fitzgerald, 1996). Mees_ures of Child Protection. Stanford-Bing Intelligence Sge, Third Revision (S-BIS; Tennan & Merrill, 1973). The S-BIS is an individually administered instrument which consists of subtests designed to measure overall level of intellectual functioning. The child’s mental age is compared with his or her chronological age, and a normalized standard score with a mean of 100 and a standard deviation of 16 is obtained to represent an overall intelligence level (IQ). ' The S-BIS has been shown to be reliable and valid and was included in the cumulative risk index based upon a score below the standardized mean for this instrument (I‘erman & Merrill, 1973). The Dimensions of Temperament Survey (DOTS-Child; Lerner, Palermo, Spiro, & Nesselroade, 1982). The DOTS-Child is a 34-item instrument which assesses child temperament on five dimensions: activity level, attention span/distractibility, approach- withdrawal/adaptability, rhythmicity, and reactivity. Items on the DOTS are answered as either “more true than false” or “more false than true” and include items such as the following: “My child moves a great deal in his/her sleep” (activity level), “Once my child is involved in a task, he/she can’t be distracted away from it” (attention span/distractibility), “It takes my child a long time to get used to new people” (approach- withdrawal/adaptability), ”My child seems to get sleepy just about the same time every ni t” (rhythmicity), and “When my child reacts to something, his/her reaction is intense” (reactivity). The dimensions of temperament contained within the DOTS-Child have been found to be reliable and valid (Lerner et al., 1982). Samples of infants, preschoolers, 45 school-aged children, and young adults have revealed reliability coeflicients on the various subscales ranging fiom .31 to .96, with reactivity being the only factor found to be consistently below .60 (Lerner et al., 1982). In studies utilizing samples of preschool-age children, Lerner et al. (1982) report that the alpha coefficients for each of the five DOTS scales are as follows: .96 for activity level, .86 for attention span/distractibility, .81 for approach-withdrawal/adaptability, .76 for rhythmicity, and .57 for reactivity. Additionally, studies have found temperament ratings on the DOTS-Child to be associated with grades, self-esteem, self-concept, depression, and peer relations in samples of children and adolescents (Windle et al., 1986). Positive temperament characteristics include low activity level, low reactivity, high attention span, high rhythmicity, and high approach/adaptability. Child temperament is defined in the current study as mother’s DOTS-Child scores. Subjects characterized by scores indicative of a positive ’ temperament (median split) on three of the five dimensions were included in the cumulative protection index (Binghanr, Zucker, & Fitzgerald, 1996). Measures of Family Environment Protection. Family Environment che (F_ES) (Moos & Moos, 1986). Family cohesion is measured using the same instrument described above for the measurement of conflict. The FES subscale, cohesion, from the relationship dimension of this instrument measures a family’s mutual supportiveness and commitment to each other. Items included in the family cohesion subscale of the FES include “Family members really help and support one another” and “There is a feeling of togetherness in our family.” This measure has been found to have acceptable test-retest reliability, ranging fiom .68 to .86 for the various subscales, as well as adequate internal consistency (Moos & Moos, 1986). Additionally, 46 the FES has been found to successfirlly discrirrrinate between distressed and normal families (Moos & Moos, 1986). Family cohesion is defined in the current study as mother’s cohesion subscale score, with inclusion in the cumulative protection index based upon a median split of this variable’s distribution in this sample (Bingham, Zucker, & Fitzgerald, 1996). TheHm 'onforM r oftheEnvir nmen-Pr h [V in (HOME; Bradley & Caldwell, 1979). The HOME is a 54-item inventory which assesses eight aspects of the social, emotional, and cognitive support available in the home environment. Two subscales of the HOME are utilized in this study to assess the quality of the home environment and maternal warmth: a safe, clean physical environment conducive to development and maternal pride, afl’ection, and warmth. Items fi'om these subscales include “Building has no potentially dangerous structural or health defect” and “Parent holds child close ten to fifteen minutes per day” and are assessed as either true of false of the home environment through direct observations and parent interviews. This instrument has been found to be a reliable and valid measure of the home environment (Bradley & Caldwell, 1979). Internal consistency for the subscales reportedly range fiom .53 to .83, with an estimate of .93 for the total scale, and concurrent and predictive validity studies revealed a significant association between scores on the HOME and child IQ (Bradley & Caldwell, 1979). Maternal warmth and a stimulating home environment were included in the cumulative protection index based upon a score above the median split of the distributions of these variables in this sample (Bingham, Zucker, & Fitzgerald, 1996) 47 Measure of Child Outcome. Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL; Achenbach & Edelbrock, 1986). The CBCL is a standardized instrument which obtains parent’s reports of a range of child behavior problems and competencies. The scoring of this instrument yields a total behavior problems scale, two broad-band subscales, and eight narrow-band subscales, as well as information regarding social competence. This study utilized the broad-band subscale, externalizing behavior problems, which are characterized by aggression and/or delinquent behaviors. Mother’s CBCL scores were used to assess child behavior problems. External validity of the CBCL has been found to be adequate, with studies finding parental CBCL ratings to be associated with independent raters’ perceptions of the child (Bird, Gould, Rubio-Stipec, Staghezza, & Canino 1991). Additionally, test-retest reliability of the CBCL has been found to range fiom .95 over a two-week period to .84 over a three-week period (Achenbach, 1991). Results Missing Date Ed Qyjliers Prior to conducting the analyses, all variables were screened for outliers and missing data. Less than two percent of the values for any variable were initially missing, and regression analyses were run on the available data based upon recruitment risk group to estimate missing data. In screening for outliers, a normal curve was superimposed on a histogram of the frequency distribution for each continuous variable, and those nonadjacent values falling outside of the normal distribution were considered to be outliers. Values adjacent to the closest. non-outlier value were assigned for each outlier in the order of original ranking, therefore maintaining the rank order of subjects for each variable. A maximum of two outlying values were altered for any variable. Descriptive Statistics The descriptive statistics for the hypothesized risk factors, child protective factors, and family protective factors are presented in Table 1, as well as the descriptive statistics for the dependent child outcome variable, mother’s report of child externalizing behavior problems. Multiple Regzession Anelyses Mein Efl‘ects Hypothesis one predicted that the risk variables would be significantly related to child externalizing behavior problems. To test this hypothesis, risk variables were entered 48 49 into a regression equation as independent variables to predict child externalizing behavior problems. The results of this regression, shown in Table 2, indicate that the overall equation was significant and explained 18% (15% adjusted R2) of the variance in the dependent variable, child externalizing behavior problems. Although only mother’s antisocial behavior checklist score contributed significantly to prediction of child externalizing behavior problems ([3=.18, senripartial r2(unique)=.023, p<.02), the hypothesized independent risk variables in combination contributed another 16% in shared variability. As many of the independent risk variables in this analysis are significantly correlated with each other, the importance of individual risk factors in the prediction of the dependent variable is diflicult to discern due to the amount of shared variance (T abachnick & Fidel], 1996). F urtherrnore, the cumulative risk index significantly predicted child externalizing behavior problems, accounting for 13% of the variance in outcome (Table 2). Hypothesis two predicted that the risk factors would be negatively related to the family environment protective factors. To test this hypothesis, separate regression analyses were conducted for each of the family environment protective factors. T1 measures of paternal alcoholism, paternal depression, paternal antisociality, maternal alcoholism, maternal depression, maternal antisociality, low SES, and marital conflict were simultaneously entered into the regression equation to predict Tl measures of family cohesion. The results of this regression, shown in Table 3, indicate that the overall equation was significant and explained 30% (28% adjusted R2) of the variance in the dependent variable, family cohesion. Although only three of the independent variables, family conflict and mother and father’s depression scores, contributed significantly to 50 prediction of family cohesion (family conflict: B=-.29, semipartial r2(unique)=.069, p<.0001; maternal depression: [3=-.37, semipartial r2(unique)=.1 l3, p<.0001; paternal depression: B=. 15, semipartial r2(unique)=.016, p<.029), the hypothesized independent risk variables in combination contributed another 10% in shared variability. Similar to the testing of hypothesis one, the importance of individual risk factors in the prediction of the dependent variable is dificult to discern due to the amount of shared variance among the independent variables. To test the relationship between the risk factors and the remaining family environment protective factors, two separate analyses utilizing the same independent variables were then conducted to predict stimulating home environment and maternal warmth as dependent variables. As indicated in Table 4, the overall equation significantly predicted stimulating home environment and explained 9% (5% adjustai R2) of the variance in the dependent variable. Paternal antisocial behavior score was the only variable that contributed significantly to the prediction of stimulating home environment (B=-.17, semipartial r2(unique)=.019, p<.039), although the independent risk variables in combination contributed another 7% in shared variability. Although the overall regression equation predicting maternal warmth was not significant, maternal depression was a significant predictor of maternal warmth (Table 5). Hypothesis three predicted a direct inverse relationship between the child protective factor variables and child outcome. To test this hypothesis, a regression analysis was conducted with child externalizing behavior problems as the dependent variable. T1 measures of child IQ and the five child temperament scales (attention, approach~withdrawal/adaptability, activity, rhythmicity, and reactivity) were 51 simultaneously entered into the regression equation to predict T2 measures of externalizing behavior problems. The results of this analysis are shown in Table 2. The overall equation was significant and explained 6% (3% adjusted R2) of the variance in the dependent variable, child externalizing behavior problems. However, none of the independent variables contributed significant unique variance to the prediction of child externalizing behavior problems. Hypothesis four predicted a direct inverse relationship between the family protective variables and child externalizing behavior problems. To test this hypothesis, Tl ' measures of family cohesion, stimulating home environment, and maternal warmth were simultaneously entered into a regression equation to predict T2 measures of externalizing behavior problems. As indicated in Table 2, the overall equation significantly predicted child externalizing behavior problems and explained 5% (4% adjusted R2) of the variance in the dependent variable. Both maternal warmth (B=-.14, semipartial r2(unique)=.012, p<.049) and family cohesion (B=~.14, semipartial r2(uniqueF.02, p<.037) contributed significantly to the prediction of child externalizing behavior problems, with the three family protective variables in combination contributing approximately another 2% in shared variability. Furthermore, the cumulative protection index, which was a summative index of both child and family environment protection, significantly predicted child externalizing behavior problems, accounting for 6% of the variance in child outcome (Table 2). W Hypothesis five predicted that child protective factors, IQ and temperament, would moderate the main effects of risk on child outcome. To assess this hypothesis, a two-step 52 regression analysis was conducted for each child protective factor. First, T1 measures of the hypothesized risk factors (paternal alcoholism, paternal depression, paternal antisociality, maternal alcoholism, maternal depression, maternal antisociality, low SES, conflict) and child IQ were simultaneously entered in step 1 into the regression equation to predict T2 measures of externalizing behavior problems. Next, the interaction terms for child IQ and each of the risk variables were entered into the regression equation (i.e., child IQ X paternal alcoholism, child IQ X paternal depression... child IQ X SES, and child IQ X conflict) as the second step. The results of this analysis are summarized in Table 6 and revealed that while there were significant main efl‘ects of the risk variables and child IQ on child externalizing behavior problems, the interaction terms did not explain a significant amount of additional variance in child outcome beyond that accounted for by the main effects (AR2=.02, AF(8,196)=.45, n<.s§). Separate two-step analyses were run testing for interactions between the risk factors and each of the five child temperament scales (attention, approach- withdrawal/adaptability, activity, rhythmicity, and reactivity). The results of these'analyses are summarized in Table 6 and reveal that while there were significant main efi‘ects of the risk variables and each dimension of child temperament on child externalizing behavior problems, the interaction terms between the child temperament dimensions and the risk variables did not explain a significant amount of additional variance in child outcome beyond that accounted for by the main effects of these variables (attention: AR2=.05, AF(8,196)= 1.73, p<.0934; approach-withdrawal/adaptability: AR2=.03, AF(8,196)= .83, p<.5779; activity: AR2=.02, AF(8,196)= .56, p<.8111; rhythrnicity: AR2=.04, AF= 1.34, 53 p<.2256; and reactivity: AR2=.04, AF(8,196)= 1.14, p<.3397). Regression analyses were then run for each risk factor individually, with a risk factor and a child protective factor in step one (i.e., paternal depression and child IQ) and the interaction term for those two variables in step two (i.e., paternal depression X IQ). The analyses indicated that child reactivity moderated the efl‘ect of maternal alcoholism on child externalizing behavior problems, in that the interaction term (child reactivity X maternal alcoholism diagnosis) explained significant additional variance in externalizing behavior problems over the main efl'ects of maternal alcoholism and child reactivity (AR2=DZ, AF(1, 210)= 5.50, p<.02), with the overall equation explaining 11% of the variance (9% adjusted R2). In order to explore the nature of the moderating efl‘ect of child reactivity, separate regression analyses investigating the relationship between maternal alcoholism (presence or absence) and child externalizing behavior problems were run for subgroups based on high and low child reactivity (median split). The results of these analyses revealed that for those children high on reactivity, maternal alcoholism was significantly related to child externalizing behavior problems (B=.28, p<.0510). However, for thoSe children characterized by low reactivity, maternal alcoholism was not significantly related to externalizing behavior problems (B=.24, p<.0027) [See Figure 4]. It was also found that child attention moderated the efi‘ect of paternal antisocial behavior on child externalizing behavior problems. The interaction term, child attention X paternal antisocial behavior, explained significant additional variance in the outcome variable over the main efl’ects of paternal antisociality and child attention (AR2=.03, AF(1,210)= 6.93, p<.009). The overall equation explained 13% of the variance in child 54 externalizing behavior problems (11% adjusted R2). Separate regression analyses exploring the relationship between paternal antisociality and child externalizing behavior problems for subgroups based on high and low child attention (median split) were then conducted. The results of these analyses revealed that for those children low on attention, paternal antisociality was significantly related to child externalizing behavior problems (B=.43, p<.0028). Although paternal antisociality was also significantly related to externalizing behavior problems for children characterized by high attention (B=.18, p<. 0221), the efl‘ect of this risk variable was significantly attenuated for this subgroup of children, as shown in Figure 5. Additionally, it was found that child approach-withdrawal/adaptability moderated the effects of maternal alcoholism on child externalizing behavior problems. The interaction temr, child approach-writhdrawal/adaptability X maternal alcoholism, explained significant additional variance in the outcome variable over the main efi‘ects of maternal alcoholism and child approach-withdrawal/adaptability (AR2=.03, AF(1,210)= 7.14, p<.008). The overall equation explained 8% of the variance in child externalizing behavior problems (8% adjusted R2). In order to explore the nature of the moderating effect of child approach-withdrawal/adaptability, separate regression analyses investigating the relationship between maternal alcoholism and child externalizing behavior problems were performed for subgroups based on high and low child approach-withdrawal/adaptability (median split). The results of these analyses revealed that for children low on approach- withdraw/adaptability, maternal alcoholism was not significantly related to child externalizing behavior problems (B=-.02, p<.8493). However, for children characterized by high approach-withdrawal/adaptability, maternal alcoholism was significantly related to 55 externalizing behavior problems (B=.40, p<.00001) [See Figure 6]. .Mrtfiatiasfliws Hypothesis six predicted that the protective family environment factors would mediate the main efl‘ects of the risk factors on child outcome. In order to test this hypothesis, the following regression analyses were conducted in accordance with the requirements outlined by Baron and Kenny (1986) regarding the determination of mediation: l) The first set of equations regressed the mediators (family cohesion, maternal warmth, and stimulating home environment, on the risk factors (hypothesis 2), revealing thatthe overall equation significantly predicted family cohesion (R2=.30, adjusted R2=.28, F(8, 205)=l 1.18, p<.0001) and stimulating home environment (R2=.09, F(8, 205)=2.51, p<.013), but not maternal warmth (R2=.05, F(8, 205)=1.27, p<.261); 2) The second equation regressed externalizing behavior problems on the risk factors (hypothesis 1), revealing that the overall equation significantly predicted externalizing behavior problems (R2=.18, F(8, 205)=5.60, p<.0001); and 3) The third equation predicted externalizing behavior problems and was conducted in two steps. The two potential mediators, family cohesion and stimulating home environment were entered in step one and the risk factors were entered in step two of the regression equation. The results of this equation revealed that the mediators (R2=.04, F(2, 211)=3.93, p<.02) and the risk factors (AR2=.15, AF(8, 203) = 4.5, p<.0001) significantly predicted externalizing behavior problems. The following conditions must be met in order to establish mediation: 1) equation 1 must reveal that the risk factors afi‘ect the mediators; 2) equation 2 must reveal that the risk factors afl‘ect externalizing behavior problems; 3) equation 3 must reveal that the 56 mediators afl‘ect externalizing behavior problems; and 4) the effects of the risk factors on externalizing behavior problems are less in the third regression equation than in the second regression equation. The first condition was not met for maternal warmth. While the first three conditions for mediation were met for both family cohesion and stimulating home environment, the fourth condition was not, as the effects of the risk factors on externalizing behavior problems were not significantly less in the third regression equation than in the second regression equation. The hypothesized mediating variables did not substantially reduce the relationship between the risk factors and child outcome, as the relationship between risk and externalizing behavior problems remained significant and was not significantly reduced by considering the effect of the hypothesized mediators, family cohesion and stimulating home environment (Rza=.15, FA(8,203)= 4.5, p<.0001 vs. R2A=.18 when the mediators were not considered). See Table 7 for the results of these analyses. Individual mediating pathways were then investigated. Regression analyses were performed for each risk factor and mediator individually (i.e., paternal antisociality and family cohesion). Two significant pathways were found. Maternal warmth and family cohesion were both found to mediate the relationship between maternal depression and child externalizing behavior problems. The results of these analyses were as follows below: 1) Family cohesion and maternal warmth were regressed on the risk factor, maternal depression, revealing that maternal depression significantly predicted family cohesion (R2=.17, adjusted R2=.17, F(l, 212)=44.64, p<.0001) and maternal warmth (R2=.02, F(l, 212)=5.16, p<.024); 2) The second equation regressed externalizing behavior problems on the risk factors, revealing that maternal depression significantly 57 predicted externalizing behavior problems (R2=.02, F(l, 212)=4.76, p<.03); and 3) Next, extemalizing behavior problems was regressed on family cohesion in step one (Rz=.02, F(l, 212)=5.3 l, p<.022) and maternal depression in step two of the regression equation (aR’=.008, AF(1,211)= 1.82, p<. 1789). Another regression analysis was then run, regressing externalizing behavior problems on maternal warmth in step one (R’=.03, F(l , 212)=5.87, p<.0162) and maternal depression in step two of the regression equation (AR2=.015, AF(1,211)= 3.41, p<.0663). Both maternal warmth and family cohesion met all conditions for mediation of maternal depression, as outlined by Baron and Kenny (1986), as the efi‘ect of maternal depression on child externalizing behavior problems was Significantly reduced when maternal warmth or family cohesion were considered. When externalizing behavior problems was regressed on family cohesion and maternal warmth in step one (Rz=.05, F(2, 211)=5.24, p<.006) and maternal depression in step two of the regression equation (AR2=.005, AF(1,210)= 1.15, p<.2840), these two variables mediated the relationship between maternal depression and child behavior problems. W According to hypothesis seven, the strength of the relationship between child externalizing behavior problems and risk variables should differ over the continuum of risk level. A cumulative risk index was computed for each participant based upon the cut-offs for each risk variable outlined in the Methods section of this paper. Neter, Wasserrnan, and Kutrler (1990) state that sometimes the regression of Y (in this case child outcome) on X (in this case cumulative risk) follows a particular linear relation in one range of X (cumulative risk) while following a different linear relation in another 58 range of X (cumulative risk). This relationship among variables earl be addressed statistieally through piecewise regression (Neter, Wasserman, & Kutner, 1990). In the current investigation, the breakpoints along the continuum of risk level at which the relationship between risk and child externalizing behavior problems changes was determined through the use of the nonlinear regression function in Systat, which allows piecewise nonlinear regression equations with unknown breakpoints to be calculated (SPSS Inc., 1996). In this way, different regression functions earl be fitted to the same data, and the piecewise nonlinear regression function in Systat estimates the point at which the regression function changes. The results of this analysis reveal that while the regression slopes calculated through this procedure were not significant, the break point estimate was signifieant (p .0494), with the slope of the relationship between risk and child externalizing behavior problems “signifieantly changing at a cumulative risk level of two [See Figure 7]. The break point identified by the piecewise nonlinear regression analysis was then utilized to divide the subjects into subpopulations based on the cumulative risk index cut-off of two. Chi-square tests for independence were utilized to explore the relationships among the levels of child externalizing behavior problems and both cumulative risk and individual risk factors among the two subpopulations. Additionally, by computing a cross-product ratio from the 2 by 2 table of values obtained from the chi-square analysis, odds ratios were calculated to describe the association between the 2 levels of each risk variable and the level of child externalizing behavior problems (Dillon & Goldstein, 1996; Tabachnick & Fidell, 1996). In this way, the increase (or decrease for ratios less than one) in the likelihood 59 of being in an outcome category given inclusion in another category (such as risk) can be computed (Dillon & Goldstein, 1996; Tabachnick & Fidell, 1996). The results of the analysis which investigated the relationship between level of externalizing behavior problems and level of risk revealed that the level of child behavior problems was significantly related to the level of risk (Xfll «F675 $309), with the high risk children being over two times as likely to have a high level of externalizing behavior problems compared with those from the low risk group (odds ratio = 2.05). Also, while high versus low levels of externalizing behavior problems were almost equally likely for the low risk group (46% high externalizing behavior problems versus 54 96 low externalizing behavior problems), high externalizing behavior problems were found in 64% of the high risk group. The relationships between levels of individual risk factors (based upon those criteria utilized for the calculation of the cumulative risk index) and the level of externalizing behavior problems were then investigated among the break-point subpopulations. Table 8 displays the results of these analyses. As can be seen, differential relationships between individual risk variables and level of child externalizing behavior problems did exist among the two subpopulations of this sample. Additionally, it was found that the presence of maternal alcoholism was significantly related to high externalizing behavior problems in the high risk group, with those children with an alcoholic mother being over two times as likely to have a high level of behavior problems than those without maternal alcoholism (odds ratio =2.28). Children in the low risk group with an alcoholic mother were approximately half as 60 likely as those without an alcoholic mother to have behavior problems (odds ratio= .47). Also, while high levels of externalizing behavior problems were only present in 30% of the low risk children with alcoholic mothers, behavior problems were found in 72 96 of the high risk group children with alcoholic mothers. Additionally, overall risk level was associated with a differential relationship between parental depression and child externalizing behavior problems. Maternal depression was associated with a one and a half times greater chance of a high level of child externalizing behavior problems in the high risk subgroup (odds ratio= 1.48), while those children of depressed mothers in the low risk group were half as likely as those whose mother was not depressed to have behavior problems (odds ratio= .48). While high levels of externalizing behavior problems were only present in 31 % of the low risk children with depressed mothers, behavior problems were found in 69% of the high risk group children with depressed mothers. Similarly, child behavior problems were over two times as likely in high risk children with depressed fathers (odds ratio=2.06), whereas paternal depression was associated with an almost equal chance of exhibiting child behavior problems in the low risk group. Furthermore, the differential relationship between the risk variables and child outcome can also be seen when separate multiple regression analyses were run for subsections of the sample, divided according to the risk level cut-off previously identified by the break point analysis. The hypothesized risk variables were entered into a regression equation as independent variables to predict child externalizing behavior problems for these two subsamples of subjects. The results of the regression analyses, shown in Table 9, indicate that while the overall equation was significant and 61 explained 17% (10% adjusted R2) of the variance in child externalizing behavior problems for those subjects with a cumulative risk index greater than two (R2 = .17, F(8, 99)=2.46, p< .0178), the risk factors do not significantly predict child outcome for those subjects with a cumulative risk index less than or equal to two (R’=.06, F(8, 97)=.76, p< .6395). Hypothesis eight predicted differential relationships between risk and protection along the continuum of risk, with these constructs becoming decreasingly independent as risk increases. Piecewise nonlinear regression was utilized to investigate the relationship between the cumulative risk index and a cumulative protection index (see Methods section for discussion of cumulative protection index). The results of this analysis did not reveal a point along the continuum of risk at which the slope of the regression function representing the relationship between risk and protection changed significantly. The correlations between the individual risk and protective variables were then investigated within the two subgroups divided according to the cumulative risk index cut-off of two. Significant differences in the relationship between several of the risk and protective variables earl be found among the two subpopulations [See Table 10]. For example, while maternal depression was significantly negatively correlated with family cohesion in the high risk group (r=-.46, 125.0001), these variables were not significantly related in the low risk group (r=-.01, 25.905). Similarly, paternal antisociality was significantly negatively correlated with a stimulating home environment in the high risk group (r=-.24, ps.014) but not in the low risk group (r=- 62 .02, 25.822). Additionally, it can be seen that the correlations between risk and protection found for the total sample are not representative of the relationship between several of these variables across the continuum of total risk [i.e. , total sample: maternal depression and family cohesion (r=-.42, 23.0001); paternal antisociality and stimulating home environment ( =-.26, 1230001)]. Chi-square analyses and odds ratios were then utilized to explore the different relationships between the protective factors and both the cumulative risk index and individual risk factors among the two subgroups. Table 11 displays the results of these analyses. High levels of cumulative protection (median split of cumulative protection index) were one quarter as likely in children from the high risk group than in children from the low risk group (Xfll m=21.4, pg .0001; odds ratio = .26). Additionally, it can be seen that there exist many differential relationships between individual risk and protective variables among the two subgroups. For example, in the high risk group, the presence of high family cohesion is less than half as likely when maternal depression is present (Xfl1 ”=5 .51, pg .018; odds ratio = .40), whereas maternal depression is almost unrelated to the presence of family cohesion in the low risk group (odds ratio = .76). Although high family cohesion can be found in 69% of the low risk group with maternal depression, only 35 % of the high risk group with maternal depression has high family cohesion present. Similarly, paternal alcoholism is almost equally likely to occur with a stimulating home environment in the low risk group (odds ratio= .73), whereas in the high risk group, the presence of paternal alcoholism makes it one quarter as likely that the home 63 environment is stimulating (odds ratio=.27). There do exist several relationships between risk and protective variables which differ across subgroups in a direction which appears contradictory to that anticipated in hypothesis eight. For example, while paternal alcoholism is equally likely to occur with high family cohesion in the low risk group, high risk group families with an alcoholic father are over three times as likely to have high levels of family cohesion (odds ratio = 3.43). However, it appears that this finding may have resulted from the significantly unequal distribution of paternal alcoholism in the high risk group due to sample recruitment procedures (high risk group: n=9 with no paternal alcoholism, n=99 with paternal alcoholism). In fact, while 90% of the low risk group with paternal alcoholism is characterized by high family cohesion, only 41% of the high risk group with alcoholic fathers has high family cohesion. Furthermore, two differential relationships based upon risk level subgroup were found in the ability of the risk factors to predict protective factors. Although the risk factors entered together into a regression equation significantly accounted for 16% of the variance in child reactivity for the high risk subgroup (R2=.16, F(8,99)=2.34, p<.023 9), these variables did not significantly predict child reactivity in the low risk subgroup (Rz=.07, F(8,97)==.9040, p<.5165). Similarly, the risk factors significantly accounted for 36% of the variance in family cohesion for the high risk group (R2=.36, F(8,99)=6.80, p<.0001) but not for the low risk group (R2=.11, F(8,97)=l.49, p<.1705). (See Tables 12 and 13). Discussion The purpose of this study was to examine the relationship between multiple risk and protective factors and the role of these variables over time in the development of externalizing behavior problems in children. Based on the literature, it was hypothesized that such child characteristics as intelligence and temperament would moderate the relationship between risk factors and externalizing behavior problems. It was also hypothesized that aspects of the family environment, such as family cohesion and maternal warmth, would mediate the relationship between risk and externalizing behavior problems. Additionally, as prior research has not adequately addressed the relationship between risk and protection, particularly in high risk samples, an important goal of the current investigation was to investigate the orthogonality of risk and protection at different points along a continuum of risk. This investigation hypothesized that there exists a differential relationship between risk and protection among risk status subpopulations, with these constructs becoming decreasingly independent as risk increases. It was therefore expected that although risk and protection would occur together on the lower end of the continuum of risk, high protection would be less likely on the high end of the continuum of risk. Additionally, it was anticipated that the predictability of child externalizing behavior problems would increase at the extremes on the continuum of risk (i.e., Bernstein & Hans, 1994). Therefore, this study investigated 65 the relationship between multiple risk and protective factors and the effects of these factors on children’s development of externalizing behavior problems over time in order to begin to explain the variations in outcome which are observed in children of alcoholics. Marn' Eggs of Risk Fgors The results of this study indicate that the hypothesized risk factors at time 1 significantly predicted child externalizing behavior problems three years later at time 2. This finding is consistent with the literature regarding the efi‘ect of parental psychopathology and environmental risk factors on children’s development of behavior problems (e.g. Fitzgerald et al., 1993; Zucker & Fitzgerald, 1991; Jansen, Fitzgerald, Ham, & Zucker, 1995; Hill & Muka, 1996; West & Prinz, 1987; Werner, 1986). While the risk factors were significantly related to child externalizing behavior problems, only 18% of the variability in child outcome was accounted for by these variables. It is therefore apparent that the incidence of child behavior problems could not be fully explained by the risk factors in this model. However, the proportion of outcome variance accounted for by the risk factors in the current investigation was fairly consistent with the literature on behavior problems in children. For example, Chassin et al. (1991) and Rubio- Stipec et al. (1991) found that risk variables explained 18% (adjusted R2) and 29% of the variability in child behavior problems, respectively. Furthermore, given the present study’s findings regarding both the direct and moderating effects of protective factors on child outcome, it is not surprising that risk alone does not account fillly for the variance in child externalizing behavior problems. The results of the present investigation are also consistent with Walker et al. ’5 (1989) conclusion that the specific efi‘ects of individual risk factors on child development 66 are often confounded due to the nonorthogonality of risk factors. Although the risk factors together significantly predicted child externalizing behavior problems, only one risk factor, maternal antisociality, contributed significant unique variance to the prediction of child behavior problems when all risk factors were considered simultaneously. However, the other risk factors together explained an additional 16% in child outcome variability. The variance shared by these highly correlated risk factors made it dificult to discern the importance of individual risk factors. This finding is not surprising in light of Kazdin and Kagan’s (1994) argument that risk factors often do not exist in isolation but rather are interrelated or appear in “packages” (p. 39). Other researchers have argued that the inability to find specific child outcomes associated with specific risk factors often results fi'om the fact that risk factors co-occur (e.g., Frick et al., 1992). Therefore, it is dificult, and perhaps artificial, to investigate the isolated efi‘ects of individual risk factors on child outcome. As a result, the cumulative risk index utilized in the current study attempted to capture the additive efi'ects of co-occurring risk factors suggested by the literature (e.g., Frick et al., 1992; Blackson & Tarter, 1994). In fact, the results of this study are consistent with the literature regarding the explanatory power of summative indexes of risk (e.g., Samerofi‘ et al., 1993; Jessor et al., 1995; Williams, Anderson, McGee, & Silva, 1990), as the cumulative risk index alone explained 13% of the variance in child externalizing behavior problems (compared to the 18% accounted for by all the risk factors considered together). However, the cumulative index of risk in this investigation did not account for a proportion of variance in outcome which is as substantial as has been evidenced in the literature. For example, Samerofi‘ et al. (1993) found that a summative 67 risk index accounted for 37% of the variance in child IQ, leading these researchers to conclude that it was not the type but rather the number of risk factors which a child encountered that determined outcome. Although the results of the present study indicate that the amount of risk encountered does impact child development, a significant proportion of variance in child outcome remained unaccounted for by the cumulative risk index. When this finding is considered in light of the moderators of risk also identified in this study, the assumption that the relationships between risk and outcome lack specificity appears not to be fully supported. As has been suggested by the literature, the risk factors in this study were also predictive of an absence of family environment protective factors. Again, the importance of individual risk factors in the prediction of the family environment protective factors remained dificult to discern due to the amount of variance shared by the risk factors (10% for family cohesion and 7% for stimulating home environment). Additionally, while the risk factors together did not significantly predict maternal warmth, maternal depression was predictive of a lack of maternal warmth. As the risk factors together were not predictive of maternal warmth, it seems possible that this variable may be a particularly important protective factor in high risk environments. This is consistent with prior research which suggests that maternal warmth is associated with better outcomes and is protective in the development of children exposed to risk (e. g., Smith & Prior, 1995; Bradley et al., 1991; Pettit & Bates, 1989; Bradley & Caldwell, 1981). However, an important implication of these findings is that the presence of maternal depression may significantly interfere with the development of mother-child warmth, therefore placing children at an increased risk for poor outcomes. 68 ME Efi‘m of Prgtfiive Fagors The results of this study are also consistent with previous studies that have found high intellectual ability, positive temperament characteristics, and family environment factors to play a protective role in the development of high-risk children (e. g., Werner, 1986; Rogosh et al., 1990; Corbo-Richert, 1994; Zucker & Gomberg, 1986; Phipps & Mulhern, 1995; Varni et al., 1990). Similar to the results found for the risk factors, most of the-protective factors did not contribute significant unique variance to the prediction of child externalizing behavior problems when considered together. Additionally, as is suggested by the literature, the cumulative protection index was predictive of child behavior problems, although it did not account for a substantial proportion of variance in outcome (Fergusson & Lynskey, 1996; Jessor et al., 1995). Overall, the findings of the current investigation regarding the relationship between protective factors and child behavior problems may suggest that protective factors are similar to risk factors in that they are interrelated or appear in “packages,” making it dificult to determine the importance of individual factors (Kazdin & Kagan, 1994, p. 39). The results of this study also appear to highlight the importance of investigating both risk and protective factors in the development of child behavior problems, as neither the risk nor the protective variables were able to fully account for the variance in child outcome. Furthermore, given the moderators efi‘ects which were also identified in this study, it may be that models which merely investigate main efi‘ects can not suficiently capture the complex relationships among risk, protection, and the development of child behavior problems. 69 Modmting Efi‘e_ct§ of thld Characteristics This study also investigated the question of whether child characteristics serve as moderators of risk, by affecting the strength and direction of the relationship between risk and outcome. The results of this study revealed that neither the interaction of the risk variables with child IQ nor the interaction of the risk factors with any of the child temperament dimensions accounted for a significant portion of variance above that accounted for by the main efi‘ects of these variables. This finding indicates that the effect of the risk factors together on child outcome is not significantly dependent upon either IQ level or child temperament status. It seems that although a good child temperament and a high IQ directly protect a child fiom poor outcomes, the adverse efi‘ect of risk factors on child behavior was not significantly attenuated by the presence of these protective child characteristics, perhaps limiting the amount of protection these characteristics can provide for children exposed to high risk. Overall, the results of this study did not support those of prior research regarding the role of child personality variables as moderators of the relationship between risk and outcome in children of alcoholics (Rogosch et al., 1990; Masten et al., 1988). Masten et al. (1988) found that child IQ moderated the relationship between stress and children’s competence at school, although child IQ was not found to moderate the efi‘ects of risk on child behavior problems in the current investigation. However, Masten et al. (1988) also reported that the role of child IQ was dependent upon the competence criterion utilized. For example, while high child IQ was found to be protective against the adverse impact of stress on children’s classroom disruptiveness, low child IQ was protective when considering classroom engagement (Masten et al., 1988). It seems that the efi‘ects of risk 70 on the outcome criterion utilized by the present study, child externalizing behavior problems, are not moderated by child intelligence. When the moderating ‘efi‘ects of individual temperament characteristics were examined in relation to the impact of a single risk factor on child outcome, three significant interaction effects were found in the current investigation. The results of these analyses revealed that for those children low on reactivity and low on approach- withdrawal/adaptability, maternal alcoholism was not significantly related to child externalizing behavior problems, whereas for those children high on reactivity and high on approach-withdrawal/adaptability, maternal alcoholism did significantly predict poor child outcome. Additionally, child attention was found to moderate the efi’ects of paternal antisociality, with the efi‘ect of this risk factor on child outcome being significantly more pronounced for those children characteiized by a low level of attention than for those children with a high level of attention. The results found regarding the moderating efi’ects of both child attention and child reactivity are consistent with the literature, which has found that an easy temperament, including the characteristics of high attentiveness and low reactivity, attenuates the relationship between risk and poor outcomes (e.g., Kyrios & Prior, 1991). The moderating efi‘ect of child approach-withdrawal/adaptability was not in the direction predicted by much of the prior research. Indeed, the hypothesized protective characteristic of high approach/adaptability (or approach behavior to novel stimuli) did not attenuate the effect of maternal alcoholism, but rather the absence of this child characteristic in the presence of maternal alcoholism resulted in more positive child outcomes. 71 Although the direction of this finding was not anticipated, it seems possible that a child temperament characterized by a tendency to approach novel situations and people may be a positive attribute in an average environment, while a temperament characterized by a tendency to withdraw fi'om unfamiliar and uncomfortable situations or people may be a more adaptive personality characteristic in the presence of maternal alcoholism. In fact, Tschann et al. (1996) found that child approachability played a similar moderating role in the relationship between high family conflict and child social withdrawal, as low approach children from high conflict families were less withdrawn than were similar children fi'om families characterized by low conflict. The opposite was found for high approach children, who were the least withdrawn in low conflict families and were more withdrawn in the presence of high conflict (T Schann et al., 1996). These researchers concluded that child approachability functions as either. a protective or a vulnerability factor, depending upon the level of family conflict to which the child is exposed (T schann et al., 1996). I When considered in light of the goodness of fit model proposed by Thomas and Chess (1981), which conceptualizes temperament not as simply good or bad but rather examines it in regard to its fit with a particular environment, it may have been ambitious to assume that specific temperament characteristics would be protective in all environments. Mgiating Effects of Fm’ly Environment Characteristics Based on prior research which has found characteristics of the family environment to be associated with positive outcomes in children who are exposed to risk factors (e. g., Smith & Prior, 1995; Seifer et al., 1992; Bradley et al., 1995), the current study not only hypothesized that protective factors would afi‘ect outcomes directly but also anticipated that family environment protective factors would account for the “how” or “why” of the 72 relationship between risk and outcome (Rogosch et al., 1990). However, the results of this study indicated that the efi‘ects of the risk factors on child behavior problems were not significantly reduced when the hypothesized mediating factors were considered. One possible explanation for this finding may be that the risk factors affect numerous aspects of the family environment and, therefore, the two mediating factors tested in this model could not suficiently account for the relationship between risk and child outcome. Perhaps a model which considered additional aspects of the family enviromnent, such as expressiveness, control, and communication among family members (e.g., Bischofi Stith, & Whitney, 1995; Grossman et al., 1992), could account for the efi‘ects of risk on child extemalizing behavior problems. However, the effects of the eight risk factors considered together in the current model account for such a substantial proportion of variance (1 8%) in child externalizing behavior problems that the significant, yet more modest, relationship between the two family environment protective factors and outcome (4%) can not suficiently account for the efi‘ects of risk. When the potential mediating efi‘ects of family environment characteristics were considered individually in relation to the impact of a single risk factor on child outcome, maternal warmth and family cohesion both were found to mediate the relationship between maternal depression and child externalizing behavior problems. In this way, maternal depression afl‘ects children indirectly, by decreasing the maternal warmth and the family cohesion present in the child’s environment. The two independent mediating pathways identified for the efi‘ects of maternal depression indicate that one risk factor can adversely impact multiple aspects of the family environment. It therefore seems that a model which does not account for numerous family environment characteristics, such as the one 73 proposed in the current investigation, will not be capable of sumciently capturing the “how” or “why” of a risk factor’s efi‘ects on child outcome. A model that accounts for a diversity of family environment characteristics may be more equipped to account for the effects of risk on child externalizing behavior problems. Efi‘gs of Risk Lag] The results of this study indicate that the slope of the regression filnction representing the relationship between risk level and child externalizing behavior problems changes at a cumulative risk level of two (see Figure 7). This study also found that the level of risk and the level of externalizing behavior problems were significantly related and that there was a greater variability in child outcome in the low risk group than in the high risk group. These findings are similar to the difl‘erential relationships between cumulative risk and child outcome among high- and low-risk subgroups identified by Bernstein and Hans (1994). Additionally, the results of this analysis replicate the findings of both Bernstein and Hans (1994) and Samerofl‘ et al. (1987) in which there were changes in the relationship between cumulative risk and child outcome following the point where there were two risk factors present. One possible way to conceptualize the change in the slope of the regression function along the continuum of risk is as a threshold of risk level, after which the accumulation of additional risk factors impacts child outcome to a greater extent than it did prior to the breakpoint in the regression function. However, Bernstein and Hans (1994) argue that while the efi‘ects of some risk factors may simply be cumulative, combinations of some specific risk factors may be especially damaging to children’s development. A particular combination of risk factors which the literature associates with 74 a significantly increased rate of behavior problems in children is parental comorbidity for psychopathology (Steinhausen et al., 1984; Hill & Muka, 1996; Chassin et al., 1991; Warner et al., 1995; Merikangas et al., 1988; Frick et al., 1991). For example, Hill and Muka (1996) found that high. risk children’s odds of having a diagnosed disorder were increased when the child lived with two alcoholic parents. Similarly, Chassin et al. (1991) found that the risk associated with parental alcoholism was mediated by co-occurring parental psychopathology and environmental stress. In the current study, it is possible that the presence of psychopathology, particularly alcoholism, in both parents may be driving the change in the slope of the regression function, as a substantial increase in the number of children with two alcoholic parents occurred following a cumulative risk index value of two. In addition, Fitzgerald et al. (1993) previously found that maternal psychopathology not only was more likely to be found in the high risk families of this sample but also was predictive of child outcome. Ellis et al. (1996) also found that the families of antisocial alcoholics in this sample had mothers with the highest levels of psychopathology and children with the greatest degree of behavior problems. Furthermore, the current investigation found a significant relationship between maternal alcoholism and child behavior problems in the high risk group, with those children exposed to maternal alcoholism being more than two times as likely as children without alcoholic mothers to exhibit externalizing behavior problems. This finding is consistent with the literature regarding the adverse efi‘ects of maternal psychopathology on child outcome (i.e., Hill & Muka, 1996; Fitzgerald et al., 1993; Werner, 1986). In fact, post-hoe X2 analyses conducted in the present investigation revealed that 75 within the total sample, parental comorbidity for alcoholism was associated with a signifieantly increased chance of a high level of externalizing behavior problems in children (p< .02; odds ratio=2.05). Similarly, within the high risk subgroup, parental comorbidity for alcoholism remained significantly related to an increased chance of a high level of externalizing behavior problems in children (n < .04; odds ratio=2.24). However, merely the presence of psychopathology in both parents was not significantly related to an increased chance of a high level of externalizing behavior problems in children. These post-hoc findings seem to provide support for the argument made by Bernstein and Hans (1994) regarding the particularly damaging efi‘ect of some combinations of specific risk factors on children’s development. Although other significant relationships between the level of individual risk factors and child behavior problems were not identified within the subgroups, several relationships between risk factors and externalizing behavior problems were found to difi'er between the risk level groups, providing further support for the importance of investigating the cumulative effects of risk in addition to combinations of specific risk factors (Bernstein & Hans, 1996). For example, parental depression was more strongly related to externalizing behavior problems in the high risk group than in the low risk group. While the results of this study highlight the importance of investigating the role of cumulative risk level in the relationship between individual risk factors and child outcome, a potential problem with comparisons across risk subgroups was also revealed, as it was found that the number of subjects characterized by a risk factor within a risk subgroup often was very small. When this occurred, it was less likely that a significant relationship between risk and outcome could be detected due to insuficient power, although important 76 information was still provided regarding the likelihood that a particular risk factor exists within a context of high or low overall risk. For example, the results of these analyses reveal that maternal antisociality did not occur within the low risk group and that all low risk children with an antisocial father exhibited a high level of externalizing behavior problems. Additionally, the results of the regression analyses for the risk level subgroups revealed that the individual risk factors were difi‘erentially predictive of child outcome contingent upon risk level. This finding is consistent with that of Seifer et al. (1992), who found difi'erential predictability of outcome based upon risk group status. It can therefore be seen that studies which do not involve high risk samples or do not account for multiple risk factors may be unable to successfully address the relationship between risk and outcome. . The slope of the regression function representing the relationship between cumulative risk level and cumulative protection level did not change at any point along the continuum of risk level, as the relationship between these two constructs was best represented by an inverse, liner regression line. It therefore appears that there did not exist a threshold of risk after which the accumulation of additional risk factors precluded the accumulation of additional protective factors to a greater extent than it did prior to the threshold. However, risk and protection were found to be negatively related, and the relationship between risk and protection differed at points along the continuum of risk for some individual variables, a finding similar to the relationships between risk and outcome found by both Bernstein and Hans (1994) and the present investigation. Significant difi‘erences between the correlations of individual risk and protective factors were found 77 between the two risk level subgroups, as many risk and protective factors were more highly inversely correlated in the high risk group than in the low risk group. While the relationship between risk and protection identified in the low risk group is consistent with Jessor et al.’s (1995) argument that these are orthogonal constructs, the increased inverse correlation between risk and protection in the high risk group suggests that risk and protection may become decreasingly orthogonal as cumulative risk increases. The efl'ect of overall risk level on the relationship between risk and protection could also be seen in difi‘erential relationships which were found involving protective variables which previously had been identified as mediators or moderators of the efl‘ects of risk on child behavior problems. For example, maternal depression was found to be significantly related to family cohesion in the high risk group (r=-.46) but not in the low risk group (r=-.01). Findings such as this involving previously identified mediating pathways and moderating efi‘ects raise the question of whether these relationships exist along the continuum of risk or only within risk level subgroups. Additionally, it was found that the ability of the risk factors taken together to predict two protective factors, child reactivity and family cohesion, significantly differed between the two risk level subgroups, as the risk factors accounted for a significant amount of the variance in these protective variables in the high risk group but not in the low risk group. This finding provides additional information which augments the results of Jessor et al. ’s (1995) study regarding the relationship between risk and protection in a sample which did not include extremes on a continuum of risk. In the current investigation, it appears that while both high and low levels of family cohesion and child reactivity coexisted with risk within an environment characterized 78 by a low level of overall risk, low levels of these protective factors were found within a context of high risk. It can be seen that the orthogonality of these constructs differed at different points along the continuum of risk, a finding which is inconsistent with Jessor et a1. ’s (1995) conclusion that high risk does not necessarily imply low protection. Additionally, the relationships between several individual risk and protective variables within the subgroups revealed that it was significantly less likely to have high levels of individual protective variables in the presence of particular risk factors in the high risk subgroup than it was in the low risk subgroup. Furthermore, the results of the analyses investigating the relationship between cumulative risk and protection level revealed that the odds were four to one against finding a high level of protection with a high level of risk. It appears that studies which do not include high risk samples (e.g., Jessor et al., 1995) may be unable to accurately determine the relationship between risk and protection, as the results of the current investigation suggest that high levels of protection are unlikely to exist within an environment characterized by high risk. There also existed several relationships between risk and protection which were statistically significant in a direction which was not anticipated. For example, it was found that maternal antisociality was inversely related to child rhythmicity in the low risk group but not in the high risk group. As prior research has found that the impact of a specific parental psychopathology can be attenuated by the presence of additional psychopathology (Moses, 1992), it may be that even low levels of maternal antisocial behavior in a context of low risk can afi’ect a child’s rhythmicity, while within a high risk context there exist numerous factors which could adversely afi‘ect a child’s rhythmicity. Similarly, low child reactivity was associated with high family conflict, particularly in the 79 high risk subgroup. Perhaps the presence of high conflict decreases the likelihood that children will react intensely to stimuli, as children may learn that an intense reaction on their part exacerbates an already conflictual environment. In this way, low child reactivity may serve an important protective function in high conflict families, a hypothesis which is supported by the literature (e.g., Tschann et al., 1995). In fact, these results are consistent with Smith and Prior’s (1995) hypothesis, based upon their discovery of independent factors for teacher and mother ratings of child temperament, that “contextual influences on expressive behaviors” may exist (p. 173). In addition, the results of these analyses again raised questions regarding mediating and moderating relationships across risk levels, as difl‘erential relationships between risk and previously identified mediating and moderating protective factors were found. Perhaps additional mediating pathways and moderating effects would have been identified in the present investigation if these relationships had been examined within risk level subgroups. Limitations of the Current Study and Directions for Future Research While it was important to control for the effects of gender and ethnicity, the entirely male, Caucasian sample utilized in this study necessarily limited the generalizability of these findings to both females and individuals from other ethnic/racial backgrounds. Future studies which investigate these relationships in more diverse populations will provide further information regarding the efi‘ect of risk and protection in the general population and may reveal differential relationships among these variables in various populations. 80 Another potential limitation of the current study was that the determination of child behavior problems relied exclusively on parent report. This is particularly problematic if difi‘erences exist between the risk groups in parental perceptions of child behavior. As previous studies have suggested that psychologically distressed parents provide ratings of their children comparable to independent sources, the extent to which this was a problem in the current investigation remains unclear (Richters & Pellegrinni, 1989). Similarly, this study also relied exclusively on self-report measures for the assessment of risk and protective factors. Further investigation of the relationship between risk, protection, and child behavior ratings obtained fiom independent sources, such as teachers, will provide information regarding the limitations of relying on parent ratings and self report for the assessment of these variables. As Masten et al. (1988) found that the relationship between stress and children’s competence was dependent upon individual characteristics as well as the competence criterion utilized, the current investigation’s use of only one area of outcome, externalizing behavior problems, may have limited the findings of this study. Perhaps studies which investigate the relationship between risk, protection, and other dimensions of outcome, such as depression and anxiety disorders, will find differential relationships among these variables. Furthermore, as mentioned previously, this study only investigated a limited number of family environment characteristics as potential mediating variables. As a result, it is likely that numerous effects of risk on the family environment, and ultimately on child outcome, were not accounted for by the present study. The inclusion of additional family environment characteristics in future studies may reveal important mediating pathways of the efi‘ects of risk on child outcome. 81 The potential protective function of external support systems, such as peers and teachers, remains another important area for future research. Although child characteristics and aspects of the family environment were investigated in this study, the third general category of protective factors specified by Garmezy (1983), external support systems, generally is not available to very young children and therefore could not be examined in the current investigation. Future studies which examine the role of external support systems in children’s lives will provide important information regarding this potential source of protection for high risk children. Based upon the results of the current investigation, it appears that future studies which investigate both mediating and moderating relationships between risk and protective variables within risk level subgroups may reveal significant protective pathways. Additionally, it would be interesting to investigate the relationship between risk and protection in the lives of older high risk children. Such an investigation could include both following the present sample over time as well as exploring the relationship between risk and protection in other samples. Finally, future studies should attempt to identify particularly damaging combinations of risk factors and continue to examine the nonspecific, cumulative effects of multiple risk factors on the development of externalizing behavior problems in children. APPENDICES APPENDIX A TABLES 82 Table 1 ".1 'ive iii or"kVari.--l Pro-.at-t'veV't-l ad-rr' {-41 Behavier Problems (n=214) Variable M D Risk Paternal Alcoholism 1.68 .47 Paternal Antisocial Behavior Score 16.28 9.95 Paternal Depression Score 2.44 2.61 Maternal Alcoholism 1.33 .47 Maternal Antisocial Behavior Score 10.24 6.56 Maternal Depression Score 2.66 2.86 Low Socioeconomic Status 340.62 126.87 Family Conflict 3.57 2.07 Family Environment Protection .. Family Cohesion 7.39 1.78 Stimulating Home Environment 6.59 .97 Maternal Warmth 5.05 1.20 Child Protection IQ 105.42 13.91 Child Activity 1.39 1.44 Child Approach-withdrawal/Adaptability 3.66 1.96 Child Attention 5.09 3.01 Child Reactivity 3.15 1.60 Child Rhythrrricity 5.53 2.24 Child Outcome Extemalizing Behavior Problems 10.89 6.52 83 Table 2 m ef Multiple Regreesion Analme Prfiieting Child Extemalizing BeMvier Prlmsn=214'M' fi‘eszikF r nP ivF Overall Individual Variables a} E a r sf— Model Risk .18 560*" Paternal Alcoholism -.09 -1.3 .006 Paternal Antisocial Behavior .11 1.4 .008 Score Paternal Depression Score .10 1.4 .008 Maternal Alcoholism .11 1.5 .009 Maternal Antisocial Behavior .18 2.4* .023 Score Maternal Depression Score .04 .5 .001 Low Socioeconomic Status .07 1.0 .004 Family Conflict . .11 1.5 .009 Family Environment Protection .05 3.95 " Family Cohesion -.14 -2.1"‘ .020 Stimulating Home -.08 -1.2 .006 Environment Maternal Warmth -.14 -2.0* .018 Child Protection .06 2.24“ IQ -.09 -1.25 .008 Child Activity .01 .19 .000 Child Adaptability -.04 -.60 .002 Child Attention -. 13 -1.69 .000 Child Reactivity .1 1 1.54 .011 Child Rhythnricity .07 1.04 .005 Cumulative Risk Index .13 303*" Cumulative Protection Index .06 123*" ‘23 05. "pg 01. "figs 001. Table 3 Sum of Multi le Re ession Anal sis Predictin Farnil Cohesion n=214: Main Effects of Risk Factors Overall Individual Variables e2 g Q r srz Model Risk .30 l l.2"”‘“'l Paternal Alcoholism -.048 -.70 .002 Paternal Antisocial Behavior -.095 -1.3 .006 Paternal Depression .145 2.20“ .017 Maternal Alcoholism -.059 -.86 .003 Maternal Antisocial Behavior -.03 -.415 .000 Maternal Depression -.366 6.76"“ .1 13 Low Socioeconomic Status -.008 -. 120 .000 Family Conflict -2.92 -4.52**“‘ .069 *95 05. “pg 01. "*pg 001. 85 Table 4 Summag of Multiple Regression Analysis Predicting Stimulating Home Environment (n=214 ): Main Efi‘ects of Risk Factors Overall Individual Variables e2 E e t i Model Risk .09 2.5“ Paternal Alcoholism -.099 -l.29 .008 Paternal Antisocial Behavior -.173 -2.07* .021 Paternal Depression Score -.049 -.652 .002 Maternal Alcoholism .120 .156 .000 Maternal Antisocial Behavior .056 .687 .002 Maternal Depression -.032 -.438 .000 Low Socioeconomic Status -.117 -l.56 .012 Family Conflict -.019 -2.65 .000 ‘23 05. "pg 01. "*9: 001. 86 Table 5 Summeg ef Multiple Regession Anelysis Predicting Maternal Wermth (n=214): Mm Efi‘eete efRiek Fegors Overall Individual Variables E F(dfl Q t i Model Risk .05 1.27 (8,205) Paternal Alcoholism -.04 -.503 .001 Paternal Antisocial Behavior .00 .025 .000 Paternal Depression .06 .823 .003 Maternal Alcoholism -.04 -.544 .001 Maternal Antisocial Behavior .00 -.017 .000 Maternal Depression -. 15 -2.05 ‘ .019 Low Socioeconomic Status .12 -1.58 .011 .000 Family Conflict -.03 -.373 ”as 05. “pg 01. "‘25 001. 87 Table 6 as fI-Iier chi .: R14 e in a 1] 1' fr V ° vl' P “1"”: Ext malizin Behavi r Problems n=214 ' Modera in f hil Prot i n Predictors AK AI _ Step 1 Risk Variables and Child IQ .18 5.06"” Step 2 Interaction terms of Child IQ X Risk Variables .02 .45 Step 1 Risk Variables and Child Activity .18 596*" Step 2 Interaction terms of Child Activity X Risk .02 .56 Variables Step 1 Risk Variables and Child Adaptability .18 496*" Step 2 Interaction terms of Child Adaptability X Risk .03 .83 Variables Step 1 Risk Variables and Child Attention .20 506*“ Step 2 Interaction terms of Child Attention X Risk .05 1.73 Variables Step 1 Risk Variables and Child Reactivity .20 567*" Step 2 Interaction terms of Child Reactivity X Risk .04 1.14 Variables Step 1 Risk Variables and Child Rhythmicity .20 5.71 *** Step 2 Interaction terms of Child Rhythmicity X Risk .04 1.34 Variables figs 05. "pg 01. “figs 001. 88 Table 7 Summm of Hierarchical Regression Analyses for Variables Predicting Child Extemalizing Behavior Problems (n=214 ): Mediating Efi‘ects ofFamily Environment Protection Predictors A_R2 A15 Step 1 Family Cohesion and Stimulating Home Environment .04 3.93“ Step 2 Risk Variables .15 .4.5"'** (Risk Variables when mediators not considered) (. 18) (5.6"*) Step 1 Family Cohesion .02 5.3 1* Step 2 Maternal Depression .008 1.82 (Maternal Depression when mediator not considered) (.02) (4.76“) Step 1 ' Maternal Warmth .03 5.87* Step 2 Maternal Depression .015 3.41 (Maternal Depression when mediator not considered) (.02) (476*) *2: 05. “pg 01. "“25 001. 89 Table 8 Sum oftheRl 'nhi Be nRik daHih khavier Preblems Agrees Risk Level Sebmpeleg‘ens V] f hild Ex rn izin High Risk (n=108) Low Riel; (n=106) Variables % w/ odds % w/ odds Risk ratiol Risk ratiol Factor Factor Paternal Alcoholism 63 .48 45 .89 Maternal Alcoholism 72 2.28“ 30 .47 Paternal Antisocial Behavior Score 65 1.09 100 - Maternal Antisocial Behavior Score 69 1.37 0 risk - Paternal Depression Score 73 2.06 50 1.18 Maternal Depression Score 69 1.48 31 .48 Low Socioeconomic Status 63 .84 50 1.24 Family Conflict 63 .88 50 1.19 *2: 05. "pg 01. "*pg 001. lgiven presence of risk factor, odds ratio equals the increased likelihood of high level of externalizing behavior problems 89 Table 8 Summm of the Relag'enships Betwgn Ri§k end a High Level ef Chile Extemalizing fiehevier Preelems Aemee Risk Qvel Sebmpelatjens High Risk (n=108) Low Risk (n=106) Variables % w/ odds % w/ odds Risk ratiol Risk ratiol Factor Factor Paternal Alcoholism 63 .48 45 .89 Maternal Alcoholism 72 2.28“ 30 .47 Paternal Antisocial Behavior Score 65 1.09 100 - Maternal Antisocial Behavior Score 69 1.37 0 risk - Paternal Depression Score 73 2.06 50 1.18 Maternal Depression Score 69 1.48 31 .48 Low Socioeconomic Status 63 .84 50 1.24 Family Conflict 63 .88 50 1.19 *9: 05. "pg 01. ““93 001. lgiven presence of risk factor, odds ratio equals the increased likelihood of high level of externalizing behavior problems 90 Table 9 Semmgy efMeltiple RegLession Analygg Predicting Child Extemalizing Behavior Problems: Main Effects of Risk Factors Within Risk Level Subggoups Risk Level B: _F_ High Risk (n=108) Risk Variables .17 2.46“ Low Risk (n=106) Risk Variables .06 .76 figs 05. "pg 01. “*2: 001. 91 .So mac: .8 was... .mo was 82 .- 88.- 22.- 82. 82.- 88.- 88.- 88.- 2.83- 288 8:5 88. 82.- 8_ _r 82.. 88.- 2.2.- 887 88.- 88. 28 33 .82. 8:. 82. 88.- 8:.- 88- 88.- 8:. 28.- 882an ease: 28. 82.- 82. 88.- 82.- 88? 88.- 88.- 88.- 88885 3538 88. .82.- 28.- 88. 88.- 88.- 88. 88.- 82.- been mm< ease: 88.- 22.- 88. 2.8.- 88.- 88.- N2 2.- 88.- E..- 288 m3. 3.53 82 7 88.- 82a.- 82. 88. 88. 2.8. 88. 82. 52282 neon: 28.- 88. e2 _. 88. 828. 2.8.- 8.8.- 82 .- 88. 582.82 3538 828 82 33 aaawfiam. :28.- 28.- 88..- 22. 88. 2.2. 88.- 88. - 6880 ans-.2 88. :88.- 88. 88. 82. .22.- 28. 82.- 8.8. 88 33 ccchoov. 88. 2.8.- 58.- 82 .- 8:. 88. a: _ .- 88.- - 88.-8am 353a: 88. 88.- 82. 82. 88. 2.8.- 88. 22.- 88. 888an 3828 88. :8.- 8:. 82.- 8:. 88. 88.- 88. 88.- 288 m3. nabs: 22. 82 7 88. .82. 88. 82.- 2.8. :88- 88. 288 mm< 3288 2.8.- 88.- 2.8.- .82 .- 88. 88. 88.- 88. 82 .- 582.82 ease: 28.- 88.- N2 _. 88. 88.- 88. 88. 88.- 82 _. 22.2.82 _aeaaa 8T5 8.2 82 .682 822 .883. 8853. 323. 8:53 .888 8880 220 85 220 86 220 9 15222 .8888 been 328m 028805 838m 882 3.. .fw .J. ...-.-..< - . . .ltt... . a .. .. .. v.44-“ :.....~. flaw—um S 033- 92 88:: 02888: 1:222: me 8:88: .8 3:8... vow-882 05 23—8 0.8. 8.28 .88.: 2...: .8 8:80.: .829 888: 88...: 9:808... 1:222: .8 25.— 3.. 2.3 .58.: 2...: 8:222: 5. .23 8803:» 805 .8 $8580.... .80 mm... .3 mm: .m: mu. 0 8...: .88 8:88 .8: .88 .8: .88 8...: 88 .2: .88 .8: .88 3.: 88 28:88 58:8 2.5.. .8: .88 .8: .88 8...: .88 88: .88 8.: .88 .8: .8. .8: .88 5: .88 .8: .88 28 5.. .8: .88 .2: .82. 5:88 .2: .88 .8: .88 .8: .88 .8: .88 3 .82 8.: .88 5.5.5: .532 .8: .88 L2: .88 .8: x... .2: .88 .8: 88 .2: .88 8...: .88 .8: .88 88.: .88 558...: .25.... mm... mm... mm... :9. mm... 8.. 8a. .8... mm... \3 0:02 \3 0:02 \3 0:07— \3 0:02 \3 0:02 \3 0:07— \3 0:02 \3 0:02 \3 0:02 0:00m mmd. 352:: 3 .82 .8: .8. 3 .82 3 .82 .8: .88 3 .8 .8: .88. 3 .88. 8.: .8. 28... mm... .25.... .8: .\.8 .8: .8. :2: .88 3 .82 .8: .8... 81:82. 3 .82 .8: .88 .8: .88 55.8.... 2552 .2: .88 .2: .88 .8: .88 .8: .88 .8: .88 .8: x8 8...: .88 E: 88 .8: .\.8 5.2.8.... .85.... 82".: .2... 33 £8,228. .2: .88 .8: .8. 3.: .8.- 88: .88 an: .88 .8: 88 2...: 8: .8: .82. 5850 2.5.. .8: .8: r8: .8... .8: .88 .2: .82. .8: .88 .8: .8... 8...: x8 .8: .88 ....: 8.... 88 33 .8: .8... .8: .8. .8: 8... .8: .88 .8: .88 .8: .88 88: .\.8 .8: 8:. .88: .88. 5.5.5: .2552 8...: .8... .8: .88 .2: .88 .8: x8 .8: .88 .8: .88 8...: .88 .8: .8: .8: .8. 5.5.5: .25.... .8: .88 .8: .88 .8: .88 8.: .88 .2: 88. .8: 8.... .8: .88 .8: .88 .8: 82. 88: mm... .2552 .8: .88 .8: 88 .8: .88 .8: 8:. .8: .82. .8: 8.... .8: .88 88: 8.8 .8: .2... .58 mm... .25.... .8: x... .8: .88 88.: .\.8 £8.88 .8: .88 .8: .82. .8: .88 .8: .88 88: .88 5.2.8.... 5.552 8.: .88 .8: .88 .8: 88 .8: .8: 88.: :8 .8: .88 .8: 8.8 .8: 88 8...: .8... 5.2.82 .55.... 82".: .5. 8... «28. 8.28: \ .88.: 02889.: 8:22—82 52>» 8. 52.8.... .85. 5.85.58... 5.5.2 :25... 5:53 5.2.... 5.8.8 2.8 2.8 2.8 2.8 2.8 o. .2552 5.5.8 2.5... 2. .2... - a. . ..... -s .u ._ .lu -. .vfiJ ...-,. .a . .«O 41.85: _ _ 038,—. 93 Table 12 Summag of Multiple Reggession Analyses Predicting Child Reactivig: Main Efi‘ects of Risk Factors Within Risk Level Subggoups Risk Level B: B High Risk (n=108) Risk Variables .16 2.34“ Low Risk (n=106) Risk Variables .0 .90 ‘95 05. "pg 01. "“23 001. 94 Table 13 Summg of Multiple Regression Analyses Predicting Family Cohesion: Main Effects of Risk Factors Within Risk Level Subgroups Risk Level 1:2 E High Risk (n=108) Risk Variables .36 6.80"” Low Risk (n=106) Risk Variables .11 1.49 *9: 05. "pg 01. “*23 001. APPENDIX B FIGURES 95 2:325 230 E... .5388; £5— 5.93qu— maEmgea—om 33853.»: a: .252 .. — 253m «Eu—co:— 3323 mes—ufiofio £8.33 HESS: comma—*8 bun—é “55835 089— mesa—=53". 328m 028805 “nogogam £55m 05830 2.5 sacs 3E8 mMm 26. €239.93 BBS“ Sum—£0855 €39.83 awe—osaanonommq _quSE Acommmoaoe 8:28 .b=a_oom:§ .Emzogoo—av awe—ogaaqoaomma 3588 22:82—52 220 O— 220 888m o>uo£o£ EEO €28.“ v6.3 .3954 “EM— ? 55:5an 2: .35 mac—a9..— ucgaaom mama—«Evin.— ES V7.2 5959m— aEmncquy— amino—=3»: - N 9...»:— _o>3 “Em .35 page: 33 2:03on Branom wEE—wEme 97 2954 «EM .3 8:55.80 2: .35 5398:...— EE :5— uooszom 9523.33— 382: confine—=3»: .. n 9...»:— Efi fix :2: 5:602 33 od 5588.5 am am?— 5953— 5322.80 o._- 98 mac—noun .8339: mama—«Egan 230 E; 83—2—82 _anua—Z 5950m— nEmueua—oy— 05 .8 33:23.: EEO u: 30...:— wnuauoeo—z .v 9.5»:— Ew__ocoo_< $5922 a0 8535 mo> oz 26.. m 3 a ll 4 w c _I \\\ I \\\\\ .2 ml. 238mm 230 \ \ \ \ \ l m.” \\\ IN? 8 3 U. B .3 w. H .8 W mm m 8 s 99 mac—acum 3330mm mus—«Eofim EEO ES b=£oomua< _aESam :ooEom augmuotflom 2: no 5653< 250 no 80...:— wEfiB—SE - m unsur— b__m_oomuc< _mEQmm ho mocommi mm; 02 :9: a 26.... l \ \ l 1.9 8:5? pilo. swe|q01d Jomeuag fiugzuewelxa mo> :9: 26.. 2:59am? 23m 26:38? .2565. B 8585 O (D ID V 1") N ‘- 0 032 1- 1- 1— u— 1- 1- 1- swelqwd Jomeuea Buszneweixa -e 2a»...— 101 mac—ac...” hogs—_om anfififioim @536on 552.3— =£mmoumom 2: mo 2.2m E ESE—«ohm .«o Ewan—3mm -2 Ban:— muouofim v—mmm mo 200852” m 0 m" D _ m 1 -T . o T Av D 0 _. C W 0 O 0 n C m 0 MW . c I 0 o 0 . lh o a m w m m C: a m w 3 w a m w m w m" a I a a w w 1:3 m o m M" .v 0 i 0 o 0 O O “Cacao—Ohm his. 0 W W 0 U LWPN O O n. 0 homxrnnmom O WENSNGHDme n- I' o I. m 9 mm 0 _ _ mm REFERENCES References Achenbach, T. (1991). Manual for the Child Behavior Chgcklist/ 4-18 and 1991 Profile. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont Department of Psychiatry. Achenbach, T. M., & Edelbrock, C. S. (1983). Mgual for the child behavior checklist and revised child behavior profile. Burlington, VT: T.M. Achenbach Publishing. Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social chhology, 51(6), 1173-1182. Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A, Garbin, M. G. (1988). Psychometric properties of the Beck Depression Inventory: Twenty-five years of evaluation. Clinical Psychology Review 8 77-100. Bernstein, V., & Hans, S. (1994). Predicting the developmental outcome of two- year-old children born exposed to methadone: Impact of social-environmental risk factors. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 23, 349-359. Bird, H. R., Gould, M. S., Rubio-Stipec, M., Staghezza, B. M., & Canino, G. (1991). Screening for childhood psychopathology in the community using the Child Behavior Checklist. Journal of the American Academy of Children and Adolescents, 3011), 116-123. Bischof, G., Stith, S., & Whitney, M. (1995). Family environments of adolescent sex offenders and other juvenile delinquents. Adolescence, 301117), 157-170. Black, A E., & Pedro-Carroll, J. (1993). Role of parent-child relationships in mediating the efl‘ects of marital disruption. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Pachiatry, 3215 1, 1019-1027. Blackson, T. C., & Tarter, R. E. (1994). Individual, family, and peer afiiliation factors predisposing to early-age onset of alcohol and drug use. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 18(4), 813-821. 103 Bolger, K. E., Patterson, C. J., Thompson, W. W., & Kupersmidt, J. B. (1995). Psychosocial adjustment among children experiencing persistent and intermittent family economic hardship. Child Development, 66, 1107-1129. Bond, C. R., & McMahon, R. J. (1984). Relationships between marital distress and child behavior problems, maternal personal adjustment, maternal personality, and maternal parenting behavior. ourn f Abn rm P ha] 93 3 348-351. Bradley, R. H., & Caldwell, B. M. (1979). Home observation for measurement of the environment: A revision of the preschool scale. Amerim 19mm! pf Meng Deficiency, 84(3 1, 235-244. Bradley, R. H., & Caldwell, B. M. (1984). The HOME inventory and family demographics. D vel men P ch 1 20 2 315-320. Bradley, R. H., & Caldwell, B. M. (1981). The HOME inventory: A validation of the preschool scale for black children. Child Developmgm, 52, 708-710. Bradley, R. H., Caldwell, B. M., Rock, S. L. (1988). Home environment and school performance: A ten-year follow-up and examination of three models of environmental action. Child Development, 59, 852-867. Bradley, R. H., Caldwell, B. M., Rock S. L., & Harris, P. T. (1986). Early home environment and the development of competence: Findings from the Little Rock Longitudinal Study. hildren’ Envir nm n u r1 3 1 10-22. Bradley, R. H., Whiteside, L., Mundfrom, D. J ., Blevins-Knabe, B., Casey, P. H., Caldwell, B. M., Kelleher, K. J ., Pope, S. K., & Barrett, K. (1995). Home environment and adaptive social behavior among premature, low birth weight children: Alternative models of environmental action. Journal pf Pediatric Psychology, 203 1, 347-362. Bradley, R. H., Whiteside, L., Mundfrom, D. J., Casey, P. H., Kelleher, K. J ., & Pope, S. K. (1994). Contribution of early intervention and early caregiving experiences to resilience in low-birthweight premature children living in poverty. Journal pf Clinipal Child Psychology, 23, 425-434. Caplan, H. D. Moses, (1996). Subtypes of alcoholic women. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. Department of Psychology, Michigan State University, East Lansing, MI. Chase-Landsdale, P. L., Wakschlag, L. S., & Brooks-Gunn, J. A. (1995). A psychological perspective on the development of caring in children and youth: The role of the family. Journal of Adolescence, 18(5 1, 515-556. 104 Chassin, L., Rogosch, F., & Barrera, M. (1991). Substance use and symptomatology among adolescent children of alcoholics. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100(4), 449-463. Corbo-Richert, B. H. (1994). Coping behaviors of young children during a chest tube procedure in the pediatric intensive care unit. Matemal-Child Nursing Journal, 22(4), 134-146. Corrao, G., Busellu, G., Valenti, M., & Lepore, A. R. (1993). Alcohol-related problems within the family and global functioning of the children: A population-based study. Social P chiat & P chiatric E idemiolo 28 6 304-308. Dadds, M. R., & Powell, M. B. (1991). The relationship of interparental conflict and global marital adjustment to aggression, anxiety, and immaturity in aggressive and nonclinic children. Journal of Abnormal Child Pachology, 19(5), 553-567. Dillon, W., & Goldstein, M. (1984). Multivariate Analysis: Methods and Applications. New York: John Wiley & Sons. Donovan, J. M. (1986). An etiologic model of alcoholism. The American Journal ofPsychiatigy, 143(1), 1-11. Downey, G., & Coyne, J. C. (1990). Children of depressed parents: An integrative review. Psychological Bulletip, 108(1), 50-76. Downey, G., & Walker, E. (1992). Distinguishing family-level and child-level influences on the development of depression and aggression in children at risk. Development and Psychopathology, 4, 81-95. Ellis, D. A., Bingham, C. R., Zucker, R. A, & Fitzgerald, H. E. (1996). Typologjcal differences in patterns of risk load and child adaptation among young alcoholic families. Unpublished manuscript, Michigan State University, East Lansing. Erel, 0., & Burman, B. (1995). Interrelatedness of marital relations and parent- child relations: A meta-analytic review. Pachological Bulletip, 118(1), 108-132. Fergusson, D. M., & Lynskey, M. T. (1996). Adolescent Resiliency to family adversity. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatg, 37(3 ), 281-292. Fitzgerald, H. E., Sullivan, L. A., Ham, H. P., Zucker, R. A., Bruckel, S., & Schneider, A. M. (1993). Predictors of behavior problems in three-year-old sons of alcoholics: Early evidence for the onset of risk. Child Development, 64, 110-123. 105 Fitzgerald, H. E., Zucker, R. A, Yang, H. (1995). Developmental systems theory and alcoholism: Analyzing patterns of variation in high-risk families. P h l f Addigp'vg Bahavipra 9(1), 8-22. Frick, P. J., Lahey, B. B., Loeber, R., Stouthamer-Loeber, M., Christ, M. An. G., & Hanson, K. (1992). Familial risk factors to oppositional defiant disorder and conduct disorder: Parental Psychopathology and maternal parenting. Journal pf Conaalting and Clinifl Pachplpgy, 60(1), 49-55. Garmezy, N. (1983). Stressors of childhood. In N. Garmezy & M. Rutter (Eds) MW (99 43-84) Baltimore MD The Johns Hopkins University Press. Garmezy, N., Masten, A S., & Tellegen, A (1984). The study of stress and competence in children: A building block for developmental psychopathology. Child Devalopment, 55, 97-111. Grizenko, N., & Fisher, C. (1992). Review of studies of risk and protective factors for psychopathology in children. Canadiap 1;)qu prsyahiam, 37, 711-721. Grossman, F. K., Beinashowitz, J., Anderson, L., Sakurai, M., Firmin, L., & Flaherty, M. (1992). Risk and resilience in young adolescents. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 21(5), 529-550. Hammen, C., Burge, D., & Stansbury, K. (1990). Relationship of mother and child variables to child outcomes in a high-risk sample: A causal modeling analysis. Dev 10 men P ch 1 6 1 24-30. Hauser, R. M. (1994). Measuring socioeconomic status in studies of child development. Child Development, 65, 1541-1545. Herrenkohl, E. C., Herrenkohl, R. C., & Egolf, B. (1994). Resilient early school- age children from maltreating homes: Outcomes in late adolescence. American Journal of Ortho chiat 64 2 301-309. Hill, S. Y., & Muka, D. (1996). Childhood psychopathology in children from families of alcoholic female probands. Jpprnal pfthe American Aaadamy of Child md Adole n P chiat 35 6 725-733. Jacob, T., & Leonard, K. (1986). Psychosocial functioning in children of alcoholic fathers, depressed fathers and control fathers. Jpprnal of Studies pn AlQhol, 47(5), 373- 380. Jansen, R. E., Fitzgerald, H. E., Ham, H. P., & Zucker, R. A. (1995). Pathways into risk: Temperament and behavior problems in three- to five-year-old sons of 106 alcoholics. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 19(2), 501-509. Jessor, R., Van Den Bos, J ., Vanderryn, J., Costa, F. M., & Turbin, M. S. (1995). Protective factors in adolescent problem behavior: Moderator efl‘ects and developmental change. Developmental Psychology, 31(6), 923-933. Johnson, J. L., Sher, K. J., & Rolf, J. E. (1991). Models of vulnerability to psychopathology in children of alcoholics. Alcohol Health & Research World, 15(1), 33- 42. Johnson, 8., & Leonard, K. E., & Jacob, T. (1989). Drinking, drinking styles and drug use in children of alcoholics, depressives and controls. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 50(5), 427-431. Jouriles, E. N., Pfifliier, L. J., & O’Leary, S. G. (1988). Marital conflict, parenting, and toddler conduct problems. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 16(2), 197-206. Katz, L. F., & Gottman, J. M. (1993). Patterns of marital conflict predict children’s internalizing and externalizing behaviors. Developmental Psychology, 29(6), 940-950. Kazdin, A. E., & Kagan, J. (1994). Models of Dysfunction in Developmental Psychopathology. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 1(1), 35-52. Kyrios, M., & Prior, M. (1991). Temperament, stress and family factors in behavioral adjustment of three- five-year-old children. In S. Chess & M. E. Hertzig (Eds), Annual Proggess in Child Psychiatgy and Child Development (pp. 285-311). New York: Brunner/Mazel Publishers. Lavigne, J. V., & Faier-Routman, J. (1993). Correlates of psychological adjustment to pediatric physical disorders: A meta-analytic review and comparison with existing models. Journal of Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics, 14(2), 117-123. Lempers, J. D., Clark-Lempers, D., & Sirnons, R. L. (1989). Economic hardship, parenting, and distress in adolescence. Child Development, 60, 25-39. Lerner, R. M., Palermo, M., Spiro, A., & Nesselroade, J. R. (1982). Assessing the dimensions of temperamental individuality across the life span: The Dimensions of Temperament Survey (DOTS). Child Development, 53, 149-159. McClelland, G., & Judd, C. (1993). Statistical difiiculties of detecting interactions and moderator efl‘ects. Psychological Bulletip, 114(2), 376-390. 107 Masten, A S., Garmezy, N., Tellegen, A, Pellegrini, D. S., Larkin, K., & Larsen, A (1988). Competence and stress in school children: The moderating efl‘ects of individual and family qualities. Joamfl pf Child Pachplogy and Pachiatg, 29(6), 745- 764. Merikangas, K. R., Prusofi‘, B. A, & Weissman, M. M. (1988). Parental concordance for afl‘ective disorders: Psychopathology in ofl‘spring. Jgrnal of Afi‘ective Digrdgs, 15(3 ), 279-290. Moos, R. H., & Moos, B. S. (1986). Famjly Enytt'p' nmant Scalp Mgpal (2nd (31. ). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press. Moses, H. D. (1992). n m e r fth r 1 ti nshi fth aJmhpl prpblema and ahild bahavior problema in a high-riak pppulatipn. Unpublished master’s thesis, Michigan State University, East Lansing. Mueller, C. W., & Parcel, T. L. (1981). Measures of socioeconomic status: Alternatives and recommendations. Child Development, 32, 13-30. Neter, J ., Wasserman, W., and Kutner, M. (1990). Applied Linear Statimcal Modals. Boston, MA: Irwin Publishing Company. Pettit, G. S., & Bates, J. E. (1989). Family interaction patterns and children’s behavior problems from infancy to 4 years. Davelopmental Pyohplogy, 25(3), 413-420. Phares, V., & Compas, B. E. (1992). The role of fathers in child and adolescent psychopathology: Make room for daddy. Pachologigal Review, 111(3 ), 387-412. Phipps, S., & Mulhem, R. K. (1995). Family cohesion and expressiveness promote resilience to the stress of pediatric bone marrow transplant: A preliminary report. Developmentfl and Behavioral Pediatriaa, 16(4), 257-263. Rak, C. F., & Patterson, L. E. (1996). Promoting resilience in at-risk children. qurnal pf Cppnsaling apd Development, 74, 368-373. Richters, J ., & Pellegrinni, D. (1989). Depressed mother’s judgments about their children: An examination of the depression-distortion hypothesis. Child Devalppment, QC), 1068-1075. Robins, L. N., Helzer, J. H., Croughan, J., & Ratclifl K. S. (1981). National Institute of Mental Health Diagnostic Interview Schedule: Its history, characteristics, and validity. Archivas pf fineral Psychiatty, 38, 381-389. Rogosch, F., Chassin, L., & Sher, K. J. (1990). Personality variables as mediators 108 and moderators of family history risk for alcoholism: Conceptual and methodological issues. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 51(4), 310-318. Rubenstein, J. L, Heeren, T., Housman, D., Rubin, C., & Stechler, G. (1989). Suicidal behavior in “normal” adolescents: Risk and protective factors. American Journal of Ortho chiat 59 1 59-71. Rubia-Stipec, M., Bird, H., Canino, G., Bravo, M., & Alegria, M. (1991). Children of alcoholic parents in the community. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 52 (1), 78- 88. Rutter, M. (1987). Psychosocial resilience and protective mechanisms. American Journal of Orthopgzchiatg, 57(3), 316-331. Samerofl‘, A. J ., & Seifer, R. ( 1983). Familial risk and child competence. Child Development, 54, 1254-1268. Sarnerofl‘, A J., Seifer, R., Baldwin, A, & Baldwin, C. (1993). Stability of intelligence fiom preschool to adolescence: The influence of social and family risk factors. Child Development, 64, 80-97. Samerofl‘, A. J., Seifer, R., Barocas, R., Zax, M., & Greenspan, S. (1987). Intelligence quotient scores of 4-year-old children: Social-environmental risk factors. Pediatrics 79 343-350. Seifer, R., Samerofi‘, A J., Baldwin, C. P., & Baldwin, A (1992). Child and family factors that ameliorate risk between 4 and 13 years of age. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatg, 31(5), 893-903. Selzer, M. L., Vinokur, A, & van Rooijen, L. (1975). A self-administered short lvfichigan Alcoholism Screening Test (SMAST). Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 36(1), 117-126. Sher, K. (1991). Children of Alcoholics: A Critical Appraisal of Theog and Research. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Sher, K., & Trull (1996). Methodological issues in psychopathology research. Annual Review of Pachology, 47, 371-400. Smith, J ., & Prior, M. (1995). Temperament and stress resilience in school-age children: A within-families study. Journal of the American Academ of Child and Adolescent Psychiatfl, 34(2), 168-179 109 SPSS, Inc. (1996). Statistics: Systat 6.0 for Windows. Chicago, IL: SPSS, Inc. Sroufe, L. A, & Rutter, M. (1984). The domain of developmental psychopathology. Child Development, 55, 17-29. Stanger, C., Achenbach, T. M., & McConaughy, S. H. (1993). Three-year'course of behavioral/emotional problems in a national sample of 4- to 16- year-olds. Predictors of signs of disturbance. Jaumg of Cpnsplting apd Clinian Payahplpgy, 61(5), 839-848. Stevens, 6., & Featherman, D. L. (1981). A revised socioeconomic index of occupational status. Saga! Sciengp Rama, 10, 364-395. Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (1996). Using Multivariata Statigiaa, New York: HarperCollins College Publishers. Terman, L. M., & Merrill, M. A (1973). Stanfprd-Binet Intelligeng Sade: Manual for tha Third Revision Form L-M. Chicago: The Riverside Publishing Company. Thomas, A, & Chess, 8. (1981). The role of temperament in the contributions of individuals to their development. In R. M. Lerner & N. A. Busch-Rossnagel (Eds), Individuals as producers of their develppment: A life-span perspective. New York: Academic Press. .. Tschann, J. M., Kaiser, P., Chesney, M. A, Alkon, A, & Boyce, W. T. (1996). Resilience and vulnerability among preschool children: F arnily functioning, temperament, and behavior problems. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent chhiatgt, 35(2), 184-192. Varni, J. W., Rubenfeld, L. A, Talbot, D., Setoguchi, Y. (1990). Family functioning, temperament, and psychologic adaptation in children with congenital or acquired limb deficiencies. Annufl Progtess in Child Psychiatty and Child Development, 279-291. Walker, E., Downey, G., & Bergman, A (1989). The efl‘ects of parental psychopathology and maltreatment on child behavior: A test of the diathesis-stress model. Child Developmant, 60, 15-24. Warner, V., Mufson, L., & Weissman, M. (1995). Ofi‘spring at high and low risk for depression and anxiety: Mechanisms of psychiatric disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child ES! Adolescent Psychiatgg, 34(6), 786-797. Weist, M. D., Freedman, A. H., Paskewitz, D. A, Proescher, E. J ., & Flaherty, L. T. (1995). Urban youth under stress: Empirical identification of protective factors. Journal pf Youth and Adolescence, 24(6), 705-720. 110 Werner, E. E. (1986). Resilient ofi'spring of alcoholics: A longitudinal study fiom birth to age 18. Jotm of Studies an Alcohpl, 47, 34-40. Werner, E. E.(l990). High-risk children in young adulthood: A longitudinal study fiom birth to 32 years. In 8. Chess & M. E. Hertzig (Eds), Annpal Prpgteas in Child chhiatly gtd Child Davelopment (pp. 180-193). New York: Brunner/Mazel Publishers. Werner, E. E. (1992). The children of Kauai: Resiliency and recovery in adolescence and adulthood. Journal of Adolagant Haalth, 13, 262-268. West, M. O., & Prinz, R. J. (1987). Parental alcoholism and childhood psychopathology. P h l ' B 11 ' 10 204-218. Williams, S., Anderson, J., McGee, R, & Silva, P. A (1990). Risk factors for behavioral and emotional disorder in preadolescent children. MAM Agdm of Child gd Adplas_<‘&nt Psyghjatg, 29(3 ), 413-419. Windle, M., Hooker, K., Lenerz, K., East, P. L., Lerner, J. V., Lerner, R. M. (1986). Temperament, perceived competence, and depression in early and late adolescents. Developmental Pacholdgy, 22, 384-3 92. Wyman, P. A, Cowen, E. L., Work, W. C., & Parker, G. R. (1991). Developmental and family milieu correlates of resilience in urban children who have experienced major life stress. Amarim ngal pf Cpmmpnig Psychplpgy, 19(3 ), 405- 426. Wyrnan, P. A, Cowen, E. L., Work, W. C., Raoof; A, Gribble, P. A, Parker, G. R, & Wannon, M. (1992). Interviews with children who experienced major life stress: Family and child attributes that predict resilient outcomes. derpaLpflhaApm Academy of Child and Adolescent nghiatg, 31(5), 904-910. Zucker, R. (1989). Is risk for alcoholism predictable? A probabilistic approach to a developmental problem. Drugs and Spciaty, 3, 69-93. Zucker, R. A. (1994). Pathways to alcohol problems and alcoholism: A developmental account of the evidence for multiple alcoholisms and for contextual contributions to risk. In R. Zucker, G. Boyd, & J. Howard (Eds), Tha Development pf Al hol Problem : Ex lorin he Bio ch 3 ci M trix f Risk (pp. 254-289). Zucker, R. A, & Fitzgerald, H. E. (1991). Early developmental factors and risk for alcohol problems. A] hol Health R s h W r 15 1 18-24. Zucker, R. A, Fitzgerald, H. E., & Moses, H. D. (1995). Emergence of alcohol 111 problems and the several alcoholisms: A developmental perspective on etiological theory and life course trajectory. In D. Cicchetti & D.J. Cohen (Eds), Developmental Psychopathology, Volume 2: Risk disorder and adaptation (pp. 677-711). New York: Wiley. Zucker, R. A, & Gomberg, E. S. L. (1984). Etiology of alcoholism reconsidered: The case for a biopsychosocial process. American chhologist, 41(7), 783-793. Zucker, R. A, & Noll, R. B. (1980). The antisocial behavior checklist. Unpublished instrument, Michigan State University, East Lansing, MI. .Zucker, R. A, Noll, R. B. ,Ham, H. P. ,Fitzgerald, H. E., & Sullivan, L. S. (1994). " and validi studies. Unpublished instrument, Michigan State University, East Lansing, MI. HIGQN STQTE UNIV. LI BRQR llgllll 2lllgl 3l gllll lgllgl llllL llglllglllgllll