ulllillllllllllllmm This is to certify that the dissertation entitled THE AFTER-SCHOOL ACTIVITIES OF FIFTH GRADERS: A STUDY OF COMMONALITY, TEACHERS' RATINGS, and ACADEMIC ACHIEVEMENT presented by CHARLES ARTHUR SHELLEY, JR. has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for ph - T) , degree in Wucation //W, éww/ Major professor James Anderson Date May 7, 1984 MSU is an Affirmative Action/Equal Opportunity Institution 0-12771 Ii MSU LIBRARIES RETURNING MATERIALS: Place in book drop to remove this checkout from your record. FINES will be charged if book is returned after the date stamped below. THE AFTER—SCHOOL ACTIVITIES OF FIFTH GRADERS: A STUDY OF COMMONALITY, TEACHERS' RATINGS AND EDUCATIVE VALUE By Charles Arthur Shelley, Jr. A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Teacher Education 1984 ABSTRACT THE AFTER—SCHOOL ACTIVITIES OF FIFTH GRADERS: A STUDY OF COMMONALITY, TEACHERS' RATINGS, AND ACADEMIC ACHIEVEMENT By Charles Arthur Shelley, Jr. It was proposed in this study to discover the common after—school activities of a specific population of fifth graders and to compare teachers' perceptions of the value of these activities to academic achievement. This was done through an open-ended survey on which students were asked to provide, in twelve categories, examples of their actual after—school activities over a four day period. These examples were subcategorized by sixty—seven activity types for purposes of more convenient analysis. Scores from the California Achievement Test given at about the same time the survey was conducted were included in the data collected. The following hypotheses were formulated for consideration and focus in the study: 1. Activity selection will be similar among students regard— less of school of attendance. 2. The relative rankings of certain after—school activities by participation levels will differ with the achievement level of the students. 3. Teachers' rating of activities will correlate positively with the ranking of activities by achievement test scores. Analysis of the data showed the range of participation levels of the activity types to vary from 97% for television viewing of all types and 96% for outdoor play activities to less than 1% for activities such as playing with dungeons and dragons gamebooks or visiting a museum. High agreement among the rank ordering of participation in activities on each of the four days surveyed indicated that similar populations were responding, although the exact number of students responding on each day varied. High agreement on the ranking by occurence of participation in activities among each quartile of achievement test performance appeared to lessen the probability that a given activity type might be used as a predictor of academic achievement. A moderately high degree of agreement on activity selection in each of twenty-three schools shows the probability of activity types being common across schools. The ordering of activity types according to average achievement test scores of participants was compared to the ratings of activity types by teachers for educative value. Teachers' ratings of educational value seemed to have little or no relationship to the rankings of activities by academic CBSt scores. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS Gratitude and special thanks are due to James Anderson, chairman of my dissertation advisory committee for his helpful recommendations, patience, and continuing encourage— ment in carrying out this study. The suggestions and guidance of other committee members Howard W. Hickey, George T. Rowan, and Arden Moon were also greatly appreciated. To the principals, teachers, and students involved in this study many thanks are extended. A special word of appreciation is extended to Robert Harrison for his invaluable assistance in the preparation of the microcomputer program used to tally the broad range of survey responses gathered in this study. Finally it is most fitting to acknowledge with love and profound gratitude the patience, assistance, and support of my wife, Marjory Shelley, and the inspiration of my children, Rebecca, Daniel, and Kathryn throughout this study. ii TABLE OF CONTENTS Chapter I. II. III. IV. INTRODUCTION 0 O O O O C O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 1 Statement of the problem Purpose of the study Hypotheses Definitions of key concepts Assumptions and limitations REVIEW OF LITERATURE . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 15 Experiential learning theory Identifying common experiential learning activities Sponsored learning Prior experience for college credit Out-of-school activities of older elementary school children Rating the educative value of activities Assessing prior experiential learning Feelings of competence and interest as an outcome Academic achievement as a correlate of activity Summary 'RESEARCH DESIGN AND PROCEDURE . . . . . . . . . . . 43 Questions to be answered Development of a survey questionnaire Definition of setting and population Accounting of data gathered from students Development and use of teacher survey Summary ANALYSIS OF RESULTS . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 52 Analysis Summary iii Review of study Findings Discussion Conclusion Implications for future research APPENDIX A: APPENDIX B: BIBLIOGRAPHY SURVEY FORMS AND LETTERS TABLES SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS iv 65 9O 97 107 4.1.a 4.1.b 4.2 4.4.a 4.6.a 4.7.3 4.1 4.4 4.5 4.6 4.7 LIST OF TABLES Popularity of Activity Categories . . . . . . . . 54 Twenty Most Popular After-school Activities . . . 55 Activities Common to All Days, at All Schools, at Every Achievement Level . . . . . . . . . . . 56 Degrees of Agreement in Rank Ordering of After— school Activities by P0pularity . . . . . . . . . 58 Ranking Battery Ranking Scores Ranking Scores Ten Top Ranking Ranking Ranking Ranking Teachers' of Top Ten Activities by Total CAT Scores . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 59 of Top Ten Activities by Reading Subtest . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 60 Ranked Activities by Teachers . . . . . . 61 of Activities by Popularity . . . . . . . 97 of Activities by Total CAT Battery Scores 99 of Activities by Reading Subtest Scores . 101 of Activities by Math Subtest Scores . . 103 Ratings of Activities . . . . . . . . . 105 CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION Statement of the Problem Just as the conscientious parent may ask his* child, "What did you do in school today?", educators might also ask their students, "What did you do after school yesterday?" The knowledge gained by significant adults, such as teachers and parents, asking these questions may prove to be a valuable aid to the child in his learning. These adults, having once been children themselves, might assume to already hold such knowledge. In addition, since they are now adults they might also presume to know what information and skills will be of most value to children in the future. Nevertheless, it might be helpful to rediscover the experiences of children from them directly and to seek to discover relationships between their current out-of—school activities and their in—school skill attainment. The rationale for undertaking this study is based on the theory that it may be more educationally efficient to organize learning on an interactive model rather *The masculine pronouns such as "he" and "his" will be used in a generic sense to indicate persons of either sex throughout this dissertation, except when referring to a particular person. 2 than a one-way delivery model of instruction. Such a model would incorporate the prior experiences of the learner in the lesson to help him understand his own behavior, transfer learning to new situations, and discover related purposes for new information and skills. It is therefore suggested as important that the educator discover what the common activities of his students are and that he explore the relationships of academic learning to present experiences. Purpose of this Study The purpose of this study is, first, to investigate whether the after—school activities of children appear to be common from school to school and across achievement levels, or whether certain activities are indicative of and distinguish among school cultures and levels of academic achievement. Secondly, the outcomes of a standard measure of school achievement will be compared with teachers' judgments of the educative value of after—school activities to establish a basis for relating after—school activity and school learning. The literature on common daily life experiences as learning experiences will be examined. The investigation will involve gathering examples of the common after—school activities of children, comparing participation in designated categories of activities to academic achievement, and examining the educative value of out—of- school activities as determined by teachers' ratings. It is expected that: 1. Activity selection will be similar from school to school. 2. Some activities will appear to distinguish high achievers from low achievers. 3. Teachers will select as most educative those activities which coincide with high academic achievement. Rationale Educational theorists and practitioners appear to support the position that familiarity with common experiences increases the capabilities of the teacher to mediate learning. Dewey, for example, predicts that the learner may perceive schooling as a waste of time if it fails to relate to his familiar experience or if it leaves him with nothing to take home from school for further application.1 Tyler lists the life experiences of the learners themselves among the basic considerations for curriculum development.2 Leichter considers the child's out—of—school experiences as an educational agenda. She suggests that the 1John Dewey, The School and Society (Chicago: University of Chicago, 1900), p. 75. 2Ralph W. Tyler, Basic Principles of Curriculum and Instruction (Chicago: University of Chicago, 1949), pp.5- 16. 4 selections a learner makes from his environment, such as television viewing, peer and family contacts, and other daily activities have an influence on subsequent behavior and educational choices.3 Schubert also states that the variety of out—of—school activities ought to be viewed as curricula separate from but contributing to the child's use of school curriculum.4 Knowledge of life experience through exhaustive description, according to Van Manen, may contribute to a teacher's pedagogical orientation.5 Bernstein suggests that: If the culture of the teacher is to become part of the consciousness of the child then the culture of the child must first be in the consciousness of the teacher.6 Along these lines, Locignigo found that observations resulting from home visits, which were anticipated and analyzed using curriculum theory frameworks, enabled him to 3Hope Jensen Leichter, Families and Communities As Educators (New York: Teachers College, Columbia University, 4William H. Schubert, "Knowledge About Out—of—School Curriculum,‘ The Educational Forum 45 (January 1981), 185— 198. 5Max Van Manen, "An Experiment in Educational Theorizing: The Utrecht School," Interchange 10 (1978— 1979):48—66. 6B. Bernstein, Class, Codes and ControlL Volume One (London: Routledge and Kegan Paul, 1971), p. 199. 5 relate better to his students and consequently teaching them became easier.7 The work of Paulo Freire in teaching reading and political awareness to illiterate adults in South America is based on investigation of their cultures' most common and most meaningful themes.8 Similarly, the French pedagogue Freinet is reported to have begun his teaching by asking his pupils to talk about their out-of—school experiences.9 Among the conclusions of the National Panel on High School and Adolescent Education is the following: Out of school learning experiences seem to offer a great potential for individual cognitive and affective developTSnt, although empirical data in this area are limited. However, in spite of the promise of enhanced learning as a result of relating out-of—school experience to school curriculum, the tendencies to naturally link these 7Michael J. Locignigo "The Home-life As Curricular Phenomena" (Masters Thesis, University of Illinois at Chicago Circle, 1979-1980),;n 72. 8Paulo Freire, Pedagogy of the Oppressed (New York: Herder and Herder, 1968), pp. 86-118 and Education for Critical Consciousness (New York: Seabury Press, 1973), pp. 48—490 9William B. Lee, "Ecole Moderne Pedagogie Freinet: Educational Bonesetters," Phi Delta Kappan 61 (January 1980): 341. IOUAL, Department of Health, Education, and Welfare, The Education of Adolescents: The Final Report and Recommendations of the National Panel on High School and Adolescent Education. Pubn. OE 76—00004 (1976), p. 48. 6 experiences may be weak as a result of perceived dis— continuity. Although the child is a link between home and school, he may be considered only a "low-powered" carrier of messages.11 Leichter writes, From an institutional perspective, a central educational question has been that of the extent of continuity or discontinuity, similarity or dissimilarity, between one setting and another, for example between the family and the school. It has been assumed that the nature and extent of similarity or dissimilarity have consequences for education, with special attention having been devoted to situations in which marked "cultural gaps" appear to exist, again, for example, between the family and the school. As Lightfoot notes, Some of the discontinuities between family and school emerge from differences in their structural properties and cultural purpose. In other words, conflicts are endemic to the very nature of the family and the school as institutions and they are experienced by all cllgildren as they traverse the path from home to school. Getzels attributes such discontinuities among home, school, and community to conflicts and discrepancies among differing role expectations and differing personalities 11Eugene Litwak and Henry J. Meyer, School, Family4_and Neighborhood: The Theory and Practice of School-Communigy Relations (New York: Columbia University Press, 1974), p. 39. 12Leichter, p. 80. 13Sara Lawrence Lightfoot, Worlds Apart: Relationships Between Families and Schools (New York: Basic Books, 1978), p. 21. within differing settings.14 Adult knowledge and influence may provide the vehicle which enables a child to overcome this discontinuity, to transfer and relate learnings from diverse situations. Although many children naturally make associations and analogies for themselves in the context of new learning, when this does not occur, assistance from adults to connect new learning to prior experience can be helpful.15 Gordon and Poze, for example, found that compensatory education students who were taught to relate concepts to their own experience demonstrated better comprehension.16 Dewey writes, . . . basing education upon personal experience may mean more multiplied and more intimate contacts between the mature and the immature than ever existed in the traditional school, and iqnsequently more, rather than less guidance by others. Educators are in a position to influence, mediate and evaluate the variety of activities a child may experience 14J. W. Getzels, "The Communities of Education," in Families and Communities as Educators, ed. Hope Jensen Leichter (New York: Teachers College, Columbia University, 1979), pp. 108—116. See also J. W. Getzels, "Socialization and Education: A Note on Discontinuities," Teachers College Record 76 (December 1974): 218-225. 15William J. J. Gordon and Tony Poze, "Learning Dysfunction and Connection Making," Psychiatric Annals 8 (March 1978): 79. 16Gordon and Poze, pp. 79—88. 17John Dewey, Experience and Education (New York: MacMillan, 1938), p. 21. both in and outside of school. They assist children in refining the observation and judgment skills which enable the children to make decisions and function more effectively. They may engage in education defined by Dewey as . . . that reconstruction and reorganization of experience which adds to the meaning of experience and which increases ability go direct the course of subsequent experiences.1 Leichter uses the term "mediation", to refer to those processes by which the family (or other institution) filters educational influences - the processes by which it screens, interprets, criticizes, reinforces, Bomplements, counteracts, refracts, and transforms.1 Elkind also used "mediation" to label a process in which interaction with adults enables a child to abstract successful approaches to learning and accomplishing tasks. In this manner, the adult is used by the child as an important part of his ability to cope and function.20 This is similar to Bronfenbrenner's proposition that The involvement of caretaker and child in progressively more complex reciprocal activity generates an emotional bond, enhanced motivation, and cognitive and manipulative skills that are mutually reinforcing to both participants, are then reflected in 18John Dewey, Democracy and Education (New York: MacMillan, 1916), p. 76. 19Leichter, pp. 32—40. See also Hope Jensen Leichter, "Some Perspectives on the Family as Educator," Teachers College Record 76 (December 1974): 213—215. 20David Elkind, "How Grown—Ups Help Children Learn," Principal 60 (May 1981): 20—24. 9 the child's competence and cooperation in other situations, Epd therefore facilitate the child's future development. Freire describes the implementation of this theory as the educator entering into dialogue with the learner and enabling him to pose problems related to his experience. This consideration of personal experience brings him to the realization that he has the power to affect and transform the d.22 Freire contrasts this approach to reality of his worl the "banking theory" of education in which the educator expects to deposit knowledge in the learner for acceptance as presented and for later retrieval if it is deemed useful.23 Conrad and Hedin found that the presence of a regularly scheduled seminar, consisting of systematic and directed reflection on an experience, proved to be the strongest single factor in explaining positive student change.24 This seems to indicate, as do other sources in the literature, that learning from experience is enhanced by interaction among learners and educators. 21Urie Bronfenbrenner, "Who Needs Parent Education?" in Families and Communities as Educators, ed. Hope Jensen Leichter (New York: Teachers College, Columbia University, 1979), p. 211. 22Freire, Pedagogy of the Oppressed, pp. 75—118. 23Freire, Pedagogy of the Oppressed, p. 59. 24Dan Conrad and Diane Hedin, Executive Summary of the Final Report of the Experiential Education Evaluation Project (St. Paul, Minnesota: Center for Youth Development and Research, University of Minnesota), p. 36. 10 So it might be concluded, as Dewey states, that: It thus becomes the office of the educator to select those things within the range of existing experience that have the promise and potentiality of presenting new problems which by stimulating new ways of observation and ggdgment will expand the area of further experience. Hypotheses It is unlikely that teachers will implement mediation of common experiences as an educational technique unless this approach is in some way connected to traditional subject matter through accepted methods of measuring educational progress, namely standardized tests and teacher judgement. The following hypotheses are offered for testing whether such a connection can easily be determined by comparing the activity choices made by students at differing levels of achievement or by comparing teachers' opinions of activities to the average test scores of participants. These hypotheses are: 1. Activity selection will be similar among students regardless of school of attendance. 2. The relative rankings of certain after—school— activities by participation levels will differ according to the achievement level of the students. 3. Teachers' rating of activities will correlate 25Dewey, Experience and Education, p. 75. ll positively with the ranking of activities by achievement test scores 0 Definitions of the key concepts The chief concepts used in this study are experience, experiential learning, prior learning, academic achievement, after-school activity, commonality and educative value. Experience, as it is used in this study, is best explained by Webster's definition "something personally encountered, undergone, or lived through"26 In this sense it is often used synonymously with the term "activity". Egperiential learning is most succinctly explained by the definition of experience as "knowledge, skill, or practice derived from direct observation of or participation in events."27 The theory of how learning occurs from experience is more thoroughly covered in definitions derived from the literature reviewed in Chapter II. Prior learning indicates that an activity occurs before it is considered as a learning activity. Prior learning as it is most often applied and explained in the literature is also covered in more detail in Chapter II along with its counterpart "sponsored learning". The out—of—school activities listed by students in this research are all 26Webster's Seventh New Collegiate Dictionary (Springfield, Mass.: G. and C. Merriam Co., 1970). 27Ibid. 12 considered as prior learning activities at the outset of this study. Academic achievement could be taken to mean all successes in learning the standard curriculum taught traditionally in the schools. As it is used for the analysis of data in this study, however, it will refer to standardized achievement test scores. After—school activity is used to mean all events in which the child participates from the ending of the school day until bedtime with the exception of meals and normal health and hygiene functions. Commonality describes the relationship among activities which are common to many students. Synonyms would include familiar, popular, and everyday. It holds this broad connotation except where it is more precisely defined in the context of data analysis in Chapter IV. Educative value, as used in this study, is the potential of an activity to be an educational or learning experience. This includes, but is not limited to, the relationship inferred between out—of—school activities and academic achievement. All outcomes or correlates of experience may be considered as indicators of the educative value of a given experience. In the analysis of data it will be used specifically to describe the rankings of after-school activities according to academic test scores and the ratings 13 of these same activities by teachers. Assumptions and Limitations The following assumptions are made in the design of this study: 1. There are relationships between the experiences of children in different settings such as the home and the school. 2. Examples provided by the students themselves will be a realistic reflection of what most students actually do after school on normal school days. 3. Teachers' judgments are a meaningful measure of the educative value of an out—of—school activity. This study is undertaken with awareness of the follow— ing limitations: 1. The survey time period is limited to four days in the late spring of the year. 2. The population under study is limited to fifth graders in one middle-sized city school district. 3. There is the potential for variability of the number of subjects responding on each of the four days. 4. There may be a variability of the number of examples given by each subject on a given day. 5. No attempt was made to control or identify individual subjects within the population by race or by sex, 14 because the focus of comparison among subgroups was defined as between schools and levels of academic achievement for an otherwise homogeneous population. CHAPTER II REVIEW OF LITERATURE Support for research into the out—of—school activities of learners as educational experiences is found in the areas of curriculum development, home environment studies, child development, motivational theory and experiential learning. This literature provides the philosophical and theoretical arguments for researching the common everyday activities of learners. It identifies a variety of common activities which might be valued as educative. It further suggests evaluation in terms of the various potential outcomes and correlates of activity type experiences. Experiential Learning Theory How the educative potential of common life experience is realized may be explained in the literature defining experiential learning. To Dewey, an experience is educative when it "creates conditions of further growtflu"28 This, according to Dewey, occurs when action is directed by some leading idea, when 28Dewey, Experience and Education, p. 36. 15 16 activity is checked by observation, and when reflection on the experience suggests ways of dealing with further experience.29 The conditions of experiential learning are further described in the model of Kolb and Fry as a cycle originating with a concrete experience which the learner observes and upon which he then reflects. Through the process of reflection the learner forms abstract concepts and general— izations which he may then decide to test in a new situation. In the testing of his ideas related to prior experience, he involves himself in a new concrete experience.30 Similarly, Coleman explains learning through experience as a sequence of steps in which one first carries out an action and then sees the effects of that action in that particular instance. The person may then relate the observed effects to a general principle which he applies to anticipate the effects of future action. The result of this process is that prior experience becomes useful in determining future behavior.31 29Dewey, Experience and Education, pp. 86-87. 30David A. Kolb and Ronald Fry, "Toward an Applied Theory of Experiential Learning", in Theories of Group Processes, ed. Gary Cooper (London and New York: John Wiley and Sons, 1975), pp. 33—37. 31James S. Coleman, "Differences Between Experiential and Classroom Learning", in Experiential Learnipgp Rationale, Characteristics and Assessment, ed. Morris T. Keeton (San Francisco: Jossey—Bass, 1976), pp. 49—61. 17 Watson writes, Structuring activities to provide for a continual progression from doing, to internalizing experiences, and then expressing them creates an effective link between the classroom and the real world,3£or in the community experience is used, not stored. Identifying common experiential learning activities The literature of experiential learning, is helpful to the research to be undertaken in this study in that it identifies educative activities outside the realm of traditional formal education. Types of experiential learning activities are generally classified into two major categories: sponsored learning and prior learning. This is more precisely defined by Willingham, who states, The definition of experiential learning is a matter of no small importance. As used here, the term refers to two types of learning. One is called variously learning through life experience, prior learning, or nonsponsored learning. It includes any type of creditable learning — through work, travel, volunteer service — that a student may have acquired independent of an educational institution and typically, prior to matriculation. A second broad category of experiential learning generally called sponsored learning, is that incorporated in institutionally sponsored programs that are designed to give students more direct experience in integrating and 32Kathryn J. Watson, The Going Places Classroom: A Community Involvement Program of Action Learningpfor Elementapy Students, Research Monograph No. 23 (Gainesville, Florida: Florida University, P. K. Yonge Laboratory School, 1977), p. 11. 18 applying knowledge - especially off-campus interpghips, service activities, work programs, and the like. ,Sponsored Learning Chickering provides a basic introduction to the potential and the problems of experiential learning and suggests possibilities for planning individualized sponsored experiential learning along the lines of field experiences in subjects such as: environmental studies, race relations, counseling theories and practice, early childhood education, criminal justice, agriculture, woodwork, mental health and travel.34 Davis, Duley, and Alexander suggest similar field experiences as part of a sponsored program: living in a subculture of American society, practical work experience in industry or a government agency, working under a skilled professional or practitioner, planning to improve interpersonal competencies, developing specific skills of interest and future use, living and working in another 33Warren W. Willingham, "Critical Issues and Basic Requirements for Assessment," in Experiential Learning; Rationale, Characteristics, and Assessment, ed. Morris T. Keeton (San Francisco: Jossey—Bass, 1976), p. 225. 34Arthur W. Chickering, Experience and Learning: An Introduction to Experiential Learning, (New Rochelle, N. Y. Change Magazine Press, 1977). 19 culture, and participating in a field research project.35 The National Panel on High School and Adolescent Education recommends the following experience—based learning situations: regular jobs, community service activities, schools-without-walls, internships in public agencies, artistic performance opportunities, "action learning" (with associated academic study), public action activities, curriculum—based learning projects involving concrete experiences, and structuring experienced-based learning opportunities for youth.36 Other examples of sponsored programs include outdoor adventure programs, career education programs, and alternative activity programs such as the following: Outward Bound is comprised of several types of adventure and challenge experiences designed to teach self- awareness and social functioning.37 Experiential—Based Career Education is an example of a career program which requires its participants to 35Robert H. Davis, John S. Duley, and Lawrence T.Alexander, Field Experience (East Lansing, Michigan: Michigan State University, 1977), pp. 10—11. 360. S., Department of Health, Education, and Welfare, The Education of Adolescents: The Final Report and Recommendations of the National Panel on High School and Adolescent Education. Pubn. 0E 76-00004 (1976), p. 48. 37Robert Godfrey, "A Review of Research and Evaluation Literature on Outward Bound and Related Educational Programs", paper presented at the Conference on Experiential Education, Estes Park, Colorado, 8—11 October, 1974. 20 learn about careers through actual involvement in work in the community.38 The "School Without Walls" enables high school students to take courses out in the community from volunteer community resource personel. Examples are the Gateway High School in New Orleans, the Parkway School in Philadelphia, and the Metro School in Chicago.39 An elementary school example of action learning associated with academic learning is the "Going Places Classroom" program in which, Students who took part in the Community Involvement Project observed and interacted with people in the community who were "coping" —— at work, at play and at home. The three r's were thus perceived not as ends in themselves, but rather48s tools which help all people cope in the real world. "Foxfire" type programs, which point out the interest in learning created by a product oriented project such as a book publication, are typified by the examples Wiggington furnishes in his series of Foxfire books and explained in the 38Thomas R. Owens; Haenn, Joseph F.; and Fehrenbacher, Harry L., ”The Use of Multiple Strategies in Evaluating an Experienced—Based Career Education Program," Journal of Research and Development in Education, 12 (Spring 1979): 35-49. 39GatewayjHigh School: "School Without Walls". (New Orleans: Orleans Parish School Board, 1971). See also Robert C. Hutchkins, "School Options in Philadelphia,Their Present and Future," Educational Leadership 32 (November 1974): 88—91. 40Watson, p. 7. 21 introductions of some.41 Prior Experience for College Credit In contrast to "sponsored programs", "prior d42, refers to experiential learning", as previously state those experiences which occur outside the auspices of an organized program or school and before recognition or credit is sought for the learning which occurs as an outcome of that experience. Literature on the assessment of prior learning seems to focus primarily on translating given experiences into credit at the college level. Examples of creditable learning experiences provided by Eckstrom in How to Get College Credit for What You Have Learned as a Homemaker and Volunteer include the acquisition of skills from experiences such as: budgeting, child care, communication, gardening, home maintenance, interior decorating, house cleaning, meal planning and preparation, pet care, sewing and needlework, and involvement with agencies or organizations in the community."3 41Eliot Wigginton, The Foxfire Book (Garden City, N.Ya Anchor Books, 1972),;uu 8-17 and Foxfire 6 (1980), pp. 7 2 42Willingham, p. 225. 43Ruth B. Eckstrom, Abigail M. Harris, and Marlaine E. Lockheed, How to Get College Credit for What You Have Learned as a Homemaker and Volunteer (Princeton, New Jersey: Educational Testing Service, 1977), pp. 5—10. 22 Likewise, Stephens College in Columbia, Missouri, lists the following categories as possible sources of credit: proficiency examinations, work experiences, volunteer activities, homemaking, recreational activities or hobbies, independent reading or attainment of licenses."4 Although college level expectations for experiential learning differ from those of the elementary school the methods used to identify learning experiences might still be useful for understanding the process of assessment. One of these methods is the compilation of a portfolio to document prior experience. Such a portfolio contains all of the evidence the student can gather to document that he has participated in and learned from a life experience. Usually this gathering of evidence begins with a listing of work, volunteer, or leisure activities which might be awarded college credit. Specific suggestions on the topics and processes of portfolio develOpment for the student interested in college credit for experiential learning are provided by Assessing Prior Learning: A CAEL Student Guide by 45 Forrest. The work of the CAEL (Cooperative Assessment of Experiential Learning) project provides the most 44"Prior Learning: A Guide to Portfolio Development," (Columbia, Missouri: Stephens College Without Walls, 1977), p. 2. "SAubrey Forrest, Assessing Prior Learning: A CAEL Student Guide (Columbia, Maryland: Cooperative Assessment of Experiential Learning, 1977). 23 comprehensive source of material on assessing prior experiential learning. Some of the major components of this project are compiled in Keeton's Experiential Learning; 46 RationaleL Characteristicsp and Assessment. Out-of—school activities of older elementary school children The aforementioned examples provide fairly typical lists of the experiences and activities which colleges and universities have considered as areas for possible credit at their level. Similar activities may also be valuable learning experiences for younger students. However, since the granting of credits in the same manner is not a function of elementary schools, the listing and evaluation of these activities for school credit is not pursued at this level. Studies in other areas of research, on the other hand, do provide rather comprehensive listings of the types of activities which might be considered typical or common for the elementary school child or early adolescent. The instruments for obtaining these listings may be formal or informal. Formal interest and preference surveys are available to assist in career guidance. Among those suggested in Euros which might be used to gather data about an individual's 46Morris T. Keeton, Experiential Learning: Rationalep Characteristics, and Assessment (San Francisco: Jossey-Bass, 1976). 24 common daily activities and interests are: the California Occupational Preference System, the Career Assessment Inventory, the Educational Interest Inventory, the Kuder General Interest Survey, the Ohio Vocational Interest Survey, the Safran Student's Interest Inventory, the Strong-Campbell Interest Inventory, and the Wide Range Interest Opinion Test."7 Contemporary Approaches to Interest Measurement reviews the Strong Vocational Interest Blank for Men, the Strong Vocational Interest Blank for Women, The Minnesota Vocational Interest Inventory, The Kuder Occupational Interest Survey, the Minnesota Importance Questionnaire, the Ohio Vocational Interest Survey, and the Work Values Inventory.48 Most of these interest surveys are not scaled for younger children. They do, however, suggest, in the choices they offer, many adult interests which may parallel or even be the outcomes of children's activities. Informal open—ended inquiries may be more direct and just as useful for obtaining information from children. Almy suggests a variety of such approaches for asking children about themselves, among them: informal 47Oscar Krisen Buros, The Eighth Mental Measurements Yearbook, Vol. 2 (Highland Park, New Jersey: The Gryphon Press, 1978), pp. 1535-1649. 48Donald G. Zytowski, ed., Contemporary Approaches to Interest Measurement (Minneapolis, University of Minnesota Press, 1973). 25 self—reports, class discussion, teacher-child conferences, casual conversations, written reports, and questionnaires."9 Likewise, Tyler recommends student interviews, parents interviews, and questionnaires among methods of studying the activities, social relations, and habits of the learners themselves.SO Formal, standardized instruments may, in fact, in some instances limit the scope of the data collected to only those correlates for which the survey was designed. An open-minded naturalistic approach is appropriate according to Guba when the main thrust of the inquiry is description to determine what is there and to discover how components are related.51 Jersild derived his information for relating self— concept to common experience from open essays using simply the broad topics of "What I Like About Myself" and "What I Dislike About Myself".52 The responses from students in fourth grade through college, as summarized by Jersild, were categorized under nineteen categories, six of which suggest 49Millie Almy, Ways of Studying Children (New York: Teachers College Press, 1959), pp. 115-131. SOTyler, pp.12-13. 51Egon G. Guba, Toward a Methodology of Naturalistic nguiry in Educational Evaluation (Los Angeles: Center for the Study of Evaluation, UCLA, 1978),]h 41. 52Arthur T. Jersild, In Search of Self (New York: Teachers College Press, 1952), p. 135. 26 activity areas: animals and pets, recreation (picnics, parties, play and sports, hobbies), ability in sports and play school (study habits), special talents (music and dance, drawing and painting, construction and mechanics, speaking, home arts and skills), and religion.53 Nias's list of recreational interests, constructed by asking children to list the activities they did in their spare time, includes: watching sport (TV and reading), children's entertainment (watching TV cartoon and reading comics), academic interests (schoolwork and watching TV news), pop music, sport, animals (looking after and watching TV or reading), art (model making, painting, drawing), romance (films, stories, and magazines), crime and horror (films and reading), science, war (TV war films and westerns and reading), and general/home interests (sewing, knitting, cooking).54 Gesell and Ilg in The Child From Five to Ten and Gesell, Ilg, and Ames in Youth, The Years from Ten to Sixteen provide a listing of activities, interests and routines as gradients of maturity. These include such areas as watching movies or TV, listening to radio and records, reading certain types of stories, comics, magazines, and newspapers, collecting, caring for pets, drawing, imaginary play, 53Jersild, pp. 135-141. 54David K. B. Nias, "The Classification and Correlates of Children's Academic and Recreational Interests," Journal of Child Psychology and Ppychiatry 20 (January 1979): 76. 27 science, building projects and working tasks.55’56 Hurlock lists the following recreational activities as typical for older children and adolescents: conversations, loafing, parties, dancing, games and sports, games of intellect, gambling, hobbies (constructions and collections), exploring, daydreaming, music, reading, movies, radio listening, and television watching.57 Dave, using home observations and parent interviews, identified the following activity involvement as part of the home environment related to academic achievement: opportunities for vocabulary enlargement (books, TV, travel, picnics, verbal interaction at home), availability and use of materials related to school learning (dictionary, encyclopedia, workbooks), indoor activities of educational value (discussions, projects), outdoor activities of educational value (visits to museum, zoo, historical places), use of TV for educational purpose, use of books, magazines, newspapers and the library, educational toys, games, and hobbies, use of complex appliances, and distribution of work 55Arnold Gesell and Frances L. Ilg, The Child from Five to Ten (New York: Harper and Brothers, 1946). 56Arnold Gesell, Frances L. Ilg, and Louise Bates Amers, Youth: The Years from Ten to Sixteen (New York: Harper and Row, 1956). 57Elizabeth B. Hurlock, Adolescent Development 4th Ed. (New York: McGraw—Hill, 1973), pp. 140—171. 28 in the home.58 Dave explains that since the purpose of his study was to investigate environmental rather than individual differences, it was necessary to exclude the child as a source of information and to concentraate on the environment in which he lives, recognizing, nevertheless that the child interacts with the educational environment in the home.59 In the research of Marjoribanks the following environ— ment variables were used as indicants of family involvement in educational activities: getting help from an older sibling on homework, playing at teaching a younger sibling, a great variety of recreational activities with parents every week, parents' knowledge and encouragement of students' hobbies, an encyclopedia set in the home with discussion of the volumes content, visits of an educational nature (concerts, museums, zoos, historic places), academic or cultural courses or lessons taken by parents of students, after-school to evening meal activity (especially reading, 60 studies, or lessons), and types and amounts of TV watching. Marjoribanks makes the observation that, 58Ravindrakumar H. Dave, "The Identification and Measurement of Environmental Process Variables That Are Related to Educational Achievement", (Ph.D. dissertation, University of Chicago, 1963), pp. 153-174. 59Ibid., p. 19. 60Kevin Marjoribanks, Families and Their Learnipg Environments: An Empirical Analysis (London: Routledge and Kegan Paul, 1979),}UL 203—230. 29 . . . children's school outcomes are explained most appropriately by examining an interactionism framework of behavior. In this . .. model it is assumed that not only do situations influence individuals, but that individuals select and subsequently affect the situations with which they interact. These lists may be indicative of what we would expect to learn from any general survey of children today concerning their after-school activities. Rating the educative value of activities Activity alone is not perceived to be a sufficient indicator of learning. Activity requires observation, reflection, and decision. Indicators of growth such as knowledge, confidence, and competence must follow so that the experience becomes useful in dealing with future situations. These resulting skills and attitudes are the learning outcomes whose measurement and relative importance determine the educative value of the activity. The literature suggests many outcomes and correlates of out—of—school activity experiences, some of which may be useful for consideration in this study. A study reported in the NASSP Bulletin of 4,000 students involved in sponsored experiential learning in 20 different school systems found over 75% agreement between teachers and students on 17 out of 24 suggested outcomes. 61Ibid., p. 2. 3O 1. Concern for fellow human beings. 2. Ability to get things done and to work smoothly with others. 3. Realistic attitudes toward other people such as the elderly, handicapped, or government officials. 4. Self—motivation to learn, participate, achieve. 5. Self—concept (sense of confidence, sense of competence, self—awareness). 6. Responsibility to group or class. 7. Risk-taking —- openness to new experiences. 8. Sense of usefulness in relation to the community. 9. Problem—solving. lO. Risk—taking -- being assertive and independent. 11. Accept consequences of my own actions. 12. Gathering and analyzing information, observing, reflecting on experience. 13. Knowledge of community organizations. 14. Responsibility for my own life. 15. Awareness of community problems. 16. Assume new, important tasks in community and school. 17. Communication skills (listening, speaképg, presenting ideas through variety of media). Programs of experiential based career education anticipate outcomes such as job related skills, interpersonal skills, personal values, and an understanding of workplace 62Dan Conrad and Diane Hedin, "Are Experiential Learning Programs Effective?" NASSP Bulletin 62 (November 1978): p. 105. 31 values such as time, cost, and impact.63’64 Gaston found that problem teenagers reported positive changes in meeting challenges, self-confidence, getting along with parents, grades in school and controlling of temper following participation in a wilderness adventure program.65 The literature describes numerous other methods and measurement instruments used to assess experiential learning outcomes and correlates. Ewart used the Tennessee Self- Concept Scale to measure the effects of outdoor adventure activities upon self-concept.66 Fletcher investigated affective changes between economically disadvantaged and advantaged sixth graders at a resident outdoor education program using the Nowicki—Strickland Locus of Control, and 63Sheila C. Gordon, "Campus and Workplace as Arenasfl' in Experiential Learning: RationaleL Characteristics and Assessment, ed. Morris T.Keeton (San Francisco: Jossey-Bass, 1976), pp. 108-118. 6"Carmen E. McLeod, Handbook for Career Specialization Program§_ (Philadelphia, PA: Research for Better Schools, Inc., 1978). 65Debra Wickstrom Gaston; Plouffe, Mary Mahoney; and Chinsky, Jack M., "An Empirical Investigation of a Wilderness Adventure Program for Teenagers: The Connecticut Wilderness School," paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the Psychological Association, Washington, DAL, 31 March 1978. 66Alan Ewart, "The Effects of Outdoor Adventure Activities Upon Self-Concept," (Master's Thesis, Eastern Michigan University, 1977). 32 the All About Myself Scale.67 Owens discusses 11 evaluation strategies for experience based career education and suggests selection criteria for planning multiple evaluation strategies.68 Staley reported research and evaluation in outdoor education measuring such variables as participant behavior, creativity, hemisphericity, attitudes and values, and comprehension and cognition.69 Staley provides a list of 77 available measurement instruments for research and evaluation in outdoor education.70 Data results from a sponsored program entitled "The Going Places Classroom' imply that high community involvement students have larger academic achievement gains than do low community involvement students.71 67Sara Ann Fletcher, "A Comparison of Affective Changes Between Economically Disadvantaged and Advantaged Sixth Graders at a Resident Outdoor Education Program," (Doctor of Recreation Dissertation, Indiana University, 1973). 68Owens, pp. 35-49. 69Frederick A. Staley, "The Research, Evaluation, and Measurement Dragons in Outdoor Education," (Paper presented at the National Outdoor Education Association Meeting, Lake Placid, N.Y., October 4—8, 1979). 7OStaley. 71Watson,p. 31. 33 Assessinngrior Experiential Learning The CAEL project recommends that following the identi— fication of life experiences, it is necessary to articulate what competencies have been acquired as a result. For example, Forrest, Knapp, and Pendergrass emphasize that the process of translating past experience into learning, competence, or achievement, and not just the production of the portfolio, is a valuable learning exercise and helps one avoid the danger of allowing activity to substitute for the 72 outcomes of experience. Knapp and Sharon in A Compendium of Assessment Techniques describe all manners of assessment techniques as they might be applied to experiential learning. These include: 1. Product assessment of creative work such as reports, diaries, drawings, or photographs. 2. Performance tests add the strong element of realism to the evaluation but can be less efficient and more costly to administer. 3. Simulations allow the assessor to observe the student in a lifelike, if not actually real, setting. 4. Essay examinations allow the student to expand on 72Aubrey Forrest, Joan E. Knapp, and Judith Pendergrass, "Tools and Methods of Evaluation," in Experiential Learning: Rationale, Characteristics, and Assessment, ed. Morris T. Keeton, (San Francisco: Jossey- Bass, 1976),;un 161—188. 34 the ideas he has acquired as a result of his experience and can be judged by degrees of merit. 5. Objective written tests are an efficient method which can focus on discrete knowledge which the assessor expects the learner will have acquired. 6. Interviews permit the flexibility of probing more deeply into the student's experience of learning.73 Also important in the evaluation of experiential learning are the functions and qualifications of the persons serving as assessors as described by Whitaker. He considers among potential assessors, the learner himself, faculty teachers, agency teachers, participant observers, and expert judges. Potential assessors are rated on their eligibility to document, identify, articulate, measure, evaluate, and transcribe the outcomes of learning from both prior experience and from sponsored programs.74 Whitaker states, Teachers are the best assessors when all characteristics are considered as a package. If only one assessor is to be used, it should be the teacher.75 73Joan Knapp and Amiel Sharon, A Compendium of Assessment Techniques (Columbia, Maryland: CAEL, 1975). 74Urban G. Whitaker, "Assessors and Their Qualifications" in Experiential Learning: RationaleL Characteristics, and Assessment ed. Morris T. Keeton (San Francisco: Jossey—Bass, 1977), pp. 189-223. 75Ibid., p. 218. 35 Feelings of competence and interest as an outcome Motivational theory explores how feelings of competence influence continuing interest in a given activity. Defining experiential learning as occurring "when changes in judgments, feelings, knowledge, or skills result for a particular person from living through an event or events," Chickering touches on a number of areas which might be considered as outcomes of experiential learning, among them moral and ethical development, intellectual development, development of interpersonal styles, sense of competence, clarification of purpose, deepening of interest, and expansion of caring.76 White laid the foundation for research on this concept with his fundamental work "Motivation Reconsidered: The Concept of Competence". In White's words, the concept of competence or effectance is evident when, the child appears to be occupied with the agreeable task of developing an effective familiarity with his environment. This involves discovering the effects he can have on the environment a9? the effects the environment will have on him. He adds, . . . it may be that the satisfaction of effectance 76Arthur W. Chickering, "Developmental Change As A Major Outcome" in Experiential Learning: RationaleL Characteristics, and Assessment, ed. Morris T. Keeton, (San Francisco: Jossey—Bass, 1976), pp. 62—107. 77Robert W. White, "Motivation Reconsidered: The Concept of Competence," Psychological Review 66 (September 1959): 321. 36 contributes significantly to those feelings of interest which often sustain us so well in day—to-day actions, particularly when the things we §re doing have continuing elements of novelty.7 White explains, We can detect the influence of sense of competence in the judgments we are constantly making, often half- consciously, about what we can and cannot do. We can step across this puddle but not the next one . .. . How do we know so well what we can and cannot do? Since in the beginning we could not do any of these things, . . .it is safe to say that we have learned it all through experience. Past actions, successful and unsuccessful, have taught us the range of our effectiveness. Sense of competence is the result of cumulative learning, and it is7§ver at work influencing the next thrust of behavior. Using White's theory as a basis, Harter has operationally defined self—esteem using the construct perceived competence which is determined by a child's perceptions of himself in the experience areas of cognitive competence, social or interpersonal competence, and physical competence, especially athletic skills.80 Such perceptions of competence from experience may be very situation specific as explained by Connell.81 Related outcomes of experiential learning, possibly the 781bid., p. 323. 79Robert W. White, "Sense of Interpersonal Competence: Two Case Studies and some Reflections on Origins," in Robert W. White, ed., The Study of Lives (New York: Atherton Press, 1966), p. 75. 80Susan Harter, "Effective Motivation Reconsideredfl' Human Development 21 (1978): 58. 81 see Harter, p. 59. 37 result of feelings of competence, are intrinsic motivation82 and continuing motivation.83 Maehr defines continuing motivation as the tendency to return to and continue working on tasks away from the instructional context in which they were presented. An example, familiar among children's common activities, is playing school at home. Others are pursuing additional research, reading, or experimenting as a result of inspiration from a school science lesson.84 Jersild discovered that self-image is often a function of very common daily experiences such as caring for pets, parties, play sports, hobbies, religion, and musical or artistic talents, as well as physical and personality traits.85 Nias investigated correlations of academic and recreational interests with personality and social variables. He found low correlations and thereby concluded that information about interests is best obtained by direct methods, and that no single influence is dominant in 82E. L. Deci, Intrinsic Motivation (New York: Plenum Press, 1975). 83Martin L. Maehr, "Continuing Motivation: An Analysis of a Seldom Considered Educational Outcome," Review of Educational Research 46 (Fall 1976): 443—462. 84Ibid., 443. 85Jersild, pp. 135—141. 38 determining interests, but that it is largely a matter of chance.86 Several older studies mentioned by S. L. Pressey and R. G. Kuhlen in Psychological Development Through the Life Span point to the implications of activities as indicators of 87 interest. Among their findings are the following: Changes in interests do change with differences in age, as pointed out by Pressey.88 Lehman and Witty suggest that bright children show a greater variation in their activities.89 Similarly, Boynton says that brighter children tend toward greater diversification in their hobbies.90 Terman found that intellectually gifted children tend to participate in more activities involving thinking or intellectual activity.91 86Nias, p. 78. 878.1“ Pressey and R.CL Kuhlen, Psyghological Development Through the Life Span (New York: Harper and Row, 1975). 881bid., p. 382. 89H. K. Lehman and P. A. Witty, The Psychology of Play Activities (New York: A. S. Barnes and Company, 1927), pp. 212—223. 90Paul L. Boynton, "The Relationship Between Children's Tested Intelligence and Their Hobby Participations," Journal of Genetic Psychology 58 (June 1941): 353-362. 91L. M. Terman, ed., Mental and Physical Traits of a Thousand Gifted Children, Genetic Studies of Genius, Vol. I (Stanford: Stanford University Press, 1925), p. 437. 39 Thorndike, in computing intercorrelations among general interests (such as sports, outdoor games, playing a musical instrument), found that certain interests go together.92 Jersild and Tasch perceive education as a vehicle to promote and develop interest.93 This is significant to this study because it suggests that relationships which may exist between school and out—of— school activity are interactive and that activity may be an outcome of learning as well as the other way around. Perhaps this is brought about in the manner Anderson has set down in five principles for promoting interest development in childhood: 1. Bring the child to see his experiences in different light by reorganizing his point of view. 2. Develop specific skills that enable the child to master particular demands. 3. Attach pleasant outcomes, through secondary devices (verbal and otherwise), to various activities. 4. Give the child good models of behavior by showing interest and enthusiasm for activities. 5. Watch particularly the incidental, casual and indirect evaluation. .. .9 92E. L. Thorndike, "The Interests of Adults: II: TAhe Interrelations of Adult Interests," Journal of Educational Psychology 26 (September-October 1935): 496—507. 93A.TL Jersild and R.J} Tasch, Children's Interests and What They Suggest for Education (New York: Teachers College, Columbia University, 1949). 94John E. Anderson, "The Relation of Attitude to Adjustment," Education 73 (December 1952): 210—218. 40 Academic achievement as a correlate of activity Cognitive skill improvement is also suggested as a possible outcome of learning in non—academic settings. As noted earlier in the study of the "Going Places Classroom", larger academic achievement gains were made by students of high community involvement.95 Foley and McGuire identified evidence of cognitive skill manifested in the street play of children, many of whom still had difficulty in classroom settings. They conclude that their observations offer support for educating to the strengths these learners exhibit in informal settings.96 The idea that commonplace life experiences correlate with school achievement is more systematically researched by Dave. Through interviews with parents to determine the nature of process variables in the home environment, he discovered that these environmental factors, including the participation of children in certain types of activities with their families, could be moderately to highly predictive of 97 academic achievement. This study has been replicated with modification, but with similar findings by Marjoribanks98, 95Watson, p. 31. 96Matthew Foley and Donald McGuire, "Cognitive Skills and Street Activity," Urban Education 16 (April 1981): 13- 36. 97Dave, pp. 116-134. 98Marjoribanks. 41 99 100 Keeves, and Ware and Garber. A parallel study on intelligence correlation with home environment factors was done by Wolf using the same sample as Dave's.101 Summary The literature was reviewed to provide an overview of the theory and practice of experiential learning and of related areas of study which value the common day-to—day experiences of students. The main points discussed in the literature which are the basis for this study are as follows: 1. Adults are important mediators of experience. 2. Learning from experience requires not only activity, but also reflection and subsequent decision—making. 3. Institutions may assist in facilitating sponsored experiential learning or in assessing and crediting prior experiential learning. 4. The typical out—of—school activities of older elementary school children may be surmised from prior 99J. P. Keeves, "The Home, the School, and Achievement in Mathematics," Science Education 59 (October 1975): 349— 460. looWilliam B. Ware and Malcolm Garber, "The Home Environment as a Predictor of School Achievement," Theory Into Practice 11 (June 1972), pp. 190—195. 101Richard Wolf, "The Measurement of Environments," in Testing Problems in Perspective, ed. Anne Anastasi (Washington, ILC.: American Council on Education, 1966), pp. 491—503. 42 research, by using processes most of which are designed for high school and college students, by observing the environment and surveying parents, or by gathering the information directly from the students themselves. 5. The educative value of an experience may be ascertained on the basis of affective outcomes such as attitudes, feelings of competence, self-concept, and interest. 6. Teachers are highly rated as assessors of the educative value of the activities of students. 7. Academic performance has been shown to be a correlate of community involvement and home environment variables in some studies. In conclusion, the literature which values out—of— school experience as educative encourages the consideration of such activities and their outcomes as indicators of growth and competence. It suggests a need for the educator or researcher to keep his information current and to probe more deeply the relationships which may exist connecting the learner's experiences both in and out of school. CHAPTER III RESEARCH DESIGN AND PROCEDURES Questions to be answered In order to investigate the relationships between school achievement and after-school activities, it is first of all necessary to identify which types of after—school activities are common enough to warrant inclusion in a comparison. Awareness of the relative commonality of participation in given types of out-of—school activities throughout the population studied and among subgroupings such as day of response, school of attendance, and level of academic achievement forms the basis for testing the first and second hypotheses of this study which are: 1. Activity selection will be similar among students regardless of school of attendance. 2. The relative rankings of certain after-school activities by participation level will differ with the achievement level of the students. The following questions were used to guide and focus information gathering and analysis pertaining to these hypotheses: 1. What are the commonplace daily activities on the 43 44 after—school agenda of a child in the fifth grade? 2. Which types of activities, if any, are found to be common among students in all schools, at all levels of achievement, and on any school day? 3. How much do the rankings of after—school activities by participation level differ according to the day of the week? 4. How much do the rankings of after—school activities by participation level differ among schools? 5. How much do the rankings of after—school activities by participation level differ with the achievement levels of students? The identification of common activities may assist educators in the construction of curriculum more relevant to the daily experience of learners. If such common experiences are found to occur with approximately the same relative frequency regardless of school of attendance, the ability to generalize such a curriculum can be supported. It is presumed that finding activity participation which appears to differentiate among achievement levels would lead to more specific research into the related factors which seem to affect this differentiation. If, on the other hand, the hypothesis cannot be supported and after—school activity participation appears to be relatively unrelated to academic achievement, then subsequent research might better be directed to exploring how educators can more effectively 45 influence the potential connectedness of school learning and common culture. The identification of common after-school activities in this study is followed by an evaluation of those activities by teachers. The importance of this information is inferred by the role adult mediation plays in experiential learning according to the literature. A comparison of evaluation measures is suggested in the third hypothesis of this study which is: 3. The rating of activities by teachers will correlate positively with the ranking of activities by achievement test scores. The procedure for testing this hypothesis is projected in the following questions: 6. What is the average standardized academic test score for students participating in a given activity? 7. What is the educative value of activities as suggested by teacher ratings? 8. How does the ranking of activities by test scores compare with the ratings of these same activities by teachers? Whether the proposed agreement is found between the average test scores of participants in an activity and the teachers' ratings of the activity or not, it will have implications for more closely examining the characteristics of an activity which led it to receive a higher rating. 46 In short, this study is designed to identify common activities in the population and subgroups of the population studied, and to compare measurement of the educative value of these activities by teachers' opinions and standardized test results. The suggested implications would be an outgrowth, not a direct finding of the study analysis. Development of a survey questionnaire It is expected that use of an open—ended survey would provide a detailed listing of a variety of commonplace activities as the students themselves are able to recollect them, which would be a different point of view than was obtained through parent interviews in previous related research. A survey is a useful method of discovering the learners' experiences from the students themselves and convenient for the practicing educator according to Almy102 and Tyler.103 The specific student survey form used in this study is found in Appendix A. On the student survey students are requested to give specific examples of their activities over a four day period which fit into these categories: watch TV, do household chores, play games indoors, play outside activities or sports, do schoolwork at home, take lessons, go 102A1my, pp. 115—131. 103Tyler, pp. 12-13. 47 to the library, go to interesting places, read for fun, look in a book for information, go to a meeting (such as scouts), work on a hobby or project. The twelve response categories were derived from several sources including the areas considered for credit in the literature on assessment of prior learning, from home activities correlating with school 104 achievement in the surveys used by Dave and Marjoribanks,105 and from listings provided by students on open—ended, pre-study surveys conducted by the researcher. These categories were not considered to be all inclusive, but nevertheless typical of common and relevant organizers for the wide range of after—school activities of the population studied. Response on pilot surveys seemed to be higher with the use of categories than without. The surveys were distributed to teachers through their principals. They were instructed to ask the students surveyed to list specific examples of activities which they had actually done on the previous day. For example, under "watch TV" the students listed the names of television shows they had watched the afternoon and evening before, or under "go interesting places" they named the store or nearby city where they went. The immense variety of specific examples thus provided by the respondents was then organized into subgroups or types where 104Dave, pp. 154—174. 105Marjoribanks, pp. 203-230. 48 responses seemed to fit most logically and consistently, though perhaps not exclusively. The outcome of this organization was an arrangement of 67 types of activities which seemed to adequately represent the specific examples provided by the subjects surveyed. Comparison with other listings of children's activities, such as are identified by Hurlock,106 Jersild,107 Nias,108 and Gesell et. al.,109 reveals selections similar to those chosen for this study. Definition of setting and population A middle-sized city in Michigan was selected which would enable the researcher to collect input from a variety of social, economic, racial, and ethnic backgrounds. This was considered significant so that the commonality of activities could be determined for a homogeneous group of students. According to the district's elementary school profile for 1981 to 1982, there were 9,558 elementary school students enrolled in the public schools. The pupil racial distribution in elementary schools at the time of this survey 106Hurlock, pp. 140-162. 107Jersild, pp. 135—141. 108Nias, p. 76. 109Gesell and Ilg, The Child from Five to Ten, pp. 359- 373 and Gesell, Ilg, and Ames, Youth: The Years from Ten to Sixteen, pp. 423—448. 49 was approximately 52.9% Black, 13.1% Hispanic, 1.3% other minorities, and 32.6% white.110 Fifth graders were selected as elementary students old enough to recall and list in written form examples of their normal day-to-day activities. This is also the approximate age of subjects used in the correlative studies of home environment and school achievement done by Dave111 and by Marjoribanks.112 Late spring was selected as the time concurrent with administration of standardized achievement tests in the school district. It is recognized that many of the students' responses could be seasonal activities and therefore it is necessary to identify the days of the year when the data were gathered. Accounting of data gathered from students The entire population of 1,256 fifth graders in the school district was requested to complete a survey for four school days (May 17-20, 1982), listing in twelve categories examples of what they did after school on those days. Response was received from 1,080 students, 862 of whom responded for all four days. Most failures to respond fully 110The Saginaw Elementary School Profile, 1981-1982, Department of evaluation Services, Saginaw Public Schools, Saginaw, Michigan. 111Dave, p. 51. 112Marjoribanks, p. 37. 50 or partially could be accounted for by legitimate absences, extended field trips, or schoolwide events. Results from the May 1982 administration of the California Achievement Test have also been collected in the form of twelve categories of total scores and subscores primarily in reading, language, and mathematics. Development and use of teacher survey Following the collection of data from students, the typical subcategories of responses from students were arranged on a survey for teachers (see Appendix A). Teachers of the students surveyed were asked to rate the subcategories of activities suggested by students according to their educative value. Specifically they were asked how highly they would recommend an activity as educational. Response options included: 0 — would not recommend, not educational; l - would recommend as entertainment, not as educational; 2 - would recommend as somewhat educational; 3‘— would recommend as educational; 4 — would recommend as highly educational. Thirty-six (36) of fifty-six (56) fifth grade teachers responded. Each activity was then rated according to the average level of educational worth accorded it by the teachers. These ratings were compared to the average test scores of participants in each of the after-school activities to see if a relationship appears to exist between teachers' ratings and test performance associated with an activity. llllll. {1 I I'll l i I. I \II 4"" I‘ 1 I'll. 1" 51 Summary It was the intent of this study to draw from the responses of the students examples of common after-school activities and to compare the commonality of these activities in different schools and at different levels of academic achievement. The selected activities were ordered according to educative value as determined by the average ranking by teachers and compared to association with school achievement as determined by the average achievement test scores of students claiming to participate in the activity. This study was undertaken to assist educators in examining their awareness of children's common after—school activities and their role and influence as significant adults and mediators of children's experiences. CHAPTER IV ANALYSIS OF RESULTS This study was undertaken to investigate the commonality and educative value of after—school activities as perceived by teachers. These data were collected by asking fifth grade students to provide, within the twelve categories of a survey, examples of their actual after-school activities over a four day period, Monday through Thursday. Following this initial collection of data, the students' responses were organized into sixty—seven subcategories and their teachers were asked to rate the educational value of each category and subcategory. Analysis of the data was used to seek answers to the following questions: 1. What are the commonplace daily activities on the after—school agenda of a child in the fifth grade? 2. Which types of activities, if any, are found to be common among students in all schools, at all levels of achievement, and on any school day? 3. How much do the rankings of after-school activities by participation level differ according to the day of the week? 0 4. How much do the rankings of after—school activities 52 53 by participation level differ among schools? 5. How much do the rankings of after-school activities by participation level differ with the achievement levels of students? 6. What is the average standardized academic test score for students participating in a given activity? 7. What is the educative value of activities as suggested by teacher ratings? 8. How does the ranking of activities by test scores compare with the ratings of these same activities by teachers? Determination of commonality began with an analysis of participation by students in the activity categories suggested by their responses. Over the four day period of the survey, 1050 students responded on one or more of the four days. The percentage of these students participating in a given type of activity is shown from highest to lowest levels of participation in table 4.1. in Appendix B. Among the twelve main categories used on the survey, television viewing of any type received the highest indication of participation with 97.24% of the students providing examples of shows they watched. This was followed by outdoor play (95.62%), household chores (88.95%), and schoolwork (79.24%) as the next most popular main categories. (see table 4.1a below) 54 TABLE 4.1.a PARTICIPATION IN ACTIVITY CATEGORIES Category Percent of pOpulation Television viewing . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 97.24% Outdoor play . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 95.62% Household chores . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 88.95% Schoolwork . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 79.24% Reading . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 64.76% Playing indoors . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 64.57% Going to a place . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 64.00% Hobby activity . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 43.43% Look in a book for information . . . . . . . . 38.76% Go to a meeting . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 22.86% Take lessons . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 19.71% Go to the library . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 19.14% Among the 67 subcategories the most common occuring activities were indoor chores (84.10%), sports (80.76%), schoolwork.(79.24%), TV movies or drama (76.57%), and TV situation comedies (76.09%). (see table 4.1J) below) 55 TABLE 4.1.b. TWENTY MOST FREQUENTLY MENTIONED AFTER—SCHOOL ACTIVITIES Activity type Percent of population Indoor chores . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 84.10% Sports . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 80.76% Schoolwork . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 79.24% Watching a TV movie or drama . . . . . . . . . 76.57% Watching a TV situation comedy . . . . . . . . 76.09% Reading a story . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 48.19% Watching TV cartoons . . . . . . . . . . . . . 36.95% Go to a store or restaurant . . . . . . . . . . 33.52% Exercise activity . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 31.90% Bike riding . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 31.71% Use a study reference book . . . . . . . . . . 28.76% Play a board game of chance . . . . . . . . . . 25.52% Play a video game . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 24.10% Visit a friend or relative . . . . . . . . . . 24.10% Play a pretending game outside . . . . . . . . 20.29% Go to the library . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 19.14% Watch a TV soap opera . . . . . . . . . . . . . 15.90% Play a pretending game inside . . . . . . . . . 14.67% Go to a park or playground . . . . . . . . . . 14.10% Outdoor chores . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 13.81% See table 4.1 in Appendix B for a complete list. A category of after—school activities was consi- dered common if activities within that category were men— tioned in the responses of students in every school, at every achievement level, and on every day of the survey. Nineteen of the sixty—seven activity subcategories derived from student responses were found to be common to all of the above specified groups. The occurrence of responses indicating participation varied from 10.1% to 84.1% of the total population in activities designated as common to all groups. 56 The occurrence of activity subcategories not termed common by virtue of their not occurring in every specified group ranged from .38% to 19.14% of the total population of respondents. Generally, as might be expected, the most frequently mentioned activities overall are also the activities common to all subgroupings. There are some exceptions to this, however, as evidenced by the activities with a lower percent of participation which are nevertheless found in every subgroup of day, school, or achievement. TABLE 4.2 ACTIVITIES COMMON TO ALL DAYS AT ALL SCHOOLS AT EVERY ACHIEVEMENT LEVEL Activity Percent of population Indoor chores . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 84.10% Sports . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 80.76% Schoolwork . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 79.24% Watch a TV movie or drama . . . . . . . . . . . 76.57% Watch a TV situation comedy . . . . . . . . . . 76.09% Read a story . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 49.19% Watch a TV cartoon . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 36.95% Go to a store or restaurant . . . . . . . . . . 33.52% Exercise . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 31.90% Bike riding . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .'. 31.71% Use a study reference book . . . . . . . . . . 28.76% Play a board game of chance . . . . . . . . . . 25.52% Play a video game . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 24.10% Visit friends or relatives . . . . . . . . . . 24.10% Play a pretending game outside . . . . . . . . 20.29% Watch a TV soap opera . . . . . . . . . . . . 15.90% Play cards . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 12.48% Watch TV news . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 12.38% Build models . . . . . . 10.10% 57 The likelihood of activity categories holding the same positions by participation level regardless of day of response, school group, or achievement quartile was explored using Kendall's coefficient of concordance. First, the agreement among days of response was analyzed. This was considered significant since there were differing numbers of respondents on each day and therefore a high degree of agreement would be an indication of population similarity. The agreement of activity rankings by frequency of occurrence among the four days was calculated to be .96. This high coefficient does indicate that the variation in the number of respondents for each day of the week had little effect on the relative rankings of activities by participation level. Secondly, in each of twenty—three elementary schools, the activity categories were ranked according to the percentage of respondents indicating participation in activities within that category. This was considered important not only because of the different school environments, but also because of the differing economic and racial compositions reflected in the schools as a consequence of their neighborhoods. The degree of agreement among all of the schools for all of the activity categories is indicated by a coefficient of concordance of .71. This is a moderately high level of agreement and appears to indicate a commonality among students' after—school activities regardless of school 58 of attendance. Next, the agreement of rankings by frequency of occurrence among quartile groupings from the results of the California Achievement Test were analyzed. This was considered as a possible indicator that activity choices could be used to predict academic achievement, if the overall agreement level was low. In fact, the degree of agreement among the four quartiles was calculated to be .93. It may therefore be concluded that the relative rankings by participation in activities does not vary greatly among students at differing levels of achievement. The after— school activity choices a student makes do not appear indicative of his level of achievement. TABLE 4.3 DEGREES OF AGREEMENT IN RANK ORDERING OF AFTER— SCHOOL ACTIVITIES BY PARTICIPATION LEVEL (Based on Kendall's coefficient of concordance) Comparison of ranking Coefficient by day .96 by school of attendance ‘ .71 by achievement level .93 To see what educative value might be suggested, categories and subcategories of after—school activities were 59 ranked according to the average California Achievement Test scores of participants in those activities. These rankings by scores on the total test battery, the reading subtest, and the math subtest are shown in tables 4.4, 4.5, and 4.6 in Appendix B. The highest 10 rankings for the test battery and reading and math subtests are shown below in tables 4.4x“ 4.5.a, and 446.3. TABLE 4.4.a RANKING OF TOP TEN ACTIVITIES BY TOTAL CAT BATTERY SCORES % of Activity CAT average population Dungeons and Dragons 188.70 .96% Reading romances 175.50 .38% Gardening 171.05 3.62% Collections 170.53 5.91% Pets 170.09 8.67% Watching TV variety show 169.80 6.10% Science as a hobby 168.00 3.14% Read for interest 167.07 5.24% Babysitting 164.66 2.76% Visit a museum 164.38 .76% 60 TABLE 4.5.a RANKING OF TOP TEN ACTIVITIES BY READING SUBTEST SCORES % of Activity Reading score p0pulation Dungeons and Dragons 59.40 .96% Reading romances 51.25 .38% Science as a hobby 50.39 3.14% Pets 50.37 8.67% Watching TV variety show 49.98 6.10% Collections 49.77 5.91% Gardening 49.34 3.62% Read for interest 49.31 5.24% Puzzles 49.25 2.29% Read magazines 49.04 5.05% TABLE 4.6.a RANKING OF TOP TEN ACTIVITIES BY MATH SUBTEST SCORES % of Activity Math score population Dungeons and Dragons 64.10 .96% Reading romances 63.50 .38% Pets 60.63 8.67% Collections 60.52 5.91% Gardening 59.11 3.62% Watching TV variety show 58.47 6.10% Watching TV game show 58.12 6.38% Read for interest 58.00 5.24% Visit a museum 58.00 .76% Go to a sporting event 56.99 7.81% 61 Teachers of the students surveyed were asked to rate the categories and subcategories of after—school activities according to their opinions of each activity's inherent educational value. The average ratings in rank order are shown in table 4.7 in Appendix B. The ten top ranked activities by teachers are shown in table 4.7Jh below. TABLE 4.7.a TEN TOP RANKED ACTIVITIES BY TEACHERS Activity Average Rating on a five point scale of O — 4 Going to the library 3.73 Use a study reference book 3.71 Visit a museum 3.64 Watch TV news 3.62 Read for interest 3.59 Science as a hobby 3.57 Read newspaper 3.54 Read a biography 3.54 Schoolwork 3.41 Music lessons 3.39 The Kendall Tau Coefficient was used to compare the ranking of activities according to teachers' ratings with the rankings according to average achievement test scores identified with these activities. The agreement between rankings according to teachers' ratings of educational value 62 and rankings according to average total battery achievement test scores was found to be .02 for all activity sub— categories, -u14 for activities designated as common, and .06 for main categories“ Common subcategories and main categories were used as comparisons because they have higher participation percentages and were therefore thought to be less subject to the variances of lower participation activities. There appears to be no strong correlation between the ordering of activities by teacher ratings and the ordering of activities by test scores. This seems to indicate that teachers could not predict school achievement using their own recommendations for educational after-school activities. Analysis of hypotheses According to the first hypothesis, it is expected that: Activity selection will be similar among students regardless of school of attendance. This hypothesis was supported by a moderately high agreement coefficient of .71 among the relative frequencies with which activities were selected in each of the schools. The school environment and the cultural and economic conditions of the neighborhoods in which the schools are located do not appear to have a differentiating effect on the common activities of students throughout the district. The second hypothesis stated that: 63 The relative rankings of certain after—school activities by participation levels will differ with the achievement level of the students. This hypothesis was not supported in that there was a high agreement coefficient of .93 among the rankings of activities by level of participation in the four quartiles of achievement on the California Achievement Test. The third hypothesis was: The rating of activities by teachers will correlate positively highly with the ranking of activities by achievement test scores. This hypothesis was also not supported. A low correlation was found between teachers' ratings and rankings according to average academic test scores. Men The activity types selected from student responses showed a wide range in terms of percentage of students participating. More than one quarter of the subcategories used to analyze the students' responses were found to be common to all schools, at all levels of achievement, and on all school days. The rankings of activity types by level of participation among days showed a very high degree of agreement. A moderately high agreement among the rankings of activities in different schools may also be indicative of the common cultural aspects of these types of activities across 64 economic conditions and school neighborhood racial composition. The agreement of rankings by level of participation was very high among the different quartiles of standardized test scores, indicating little evidence to support activity choice as a predictor of academic achievement. The ordering of activity categories and subcategories by academic test achievement may provide some insight into relationships between activity choice and academic achievement. However, extreme scores, both high and low, may also be affected by the small number of responses they represent. Teachers' ratings of educational value seem to have little or no relationship to the rankings of activities by academic test scores. Analysis of the data collected did provide answers to the proposed questions regarding commonality of after—school activities and their educative value as defined in this study design. CHAPTER V SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS Review of Study Previous research provides strong theoretical support for educators knowing more about the common experiences of learners as a basis for further learning. In practice, prior experience has been considered for accreditation at the college level and sponsored experiential learning has been implemented through adventure and career programs at the high school and college levels. However, evidence of applying experiential learning theory to evaluate and use the out—of- school experiences of students in the upper elementary grades is sparse. It was proposed in this study to discover the common after—school activities of a specific population of fifth graders and to compare teachers' perceptions of the values of these activities to determine how activity selection is related to academic achievement. This was done through an Open—ended survey on which students were asked to provide in twelve categories examples of their actual after-school activities over a four day period. These examples were subcategorized by sixty—seven activity types for purposes of 65 66 more convenient analysis. Scores from the California Achievement Test given at about the same time the survey was conducted were included in the data collected. Analysis of the data showed the range of participation in the activity types. High agreement among the rank ordering of activities by participation level on each of the four days surveyed indicated that similar populations were responding, although the exact number of students responding on each day varied. High agreement on the ranking of activities by participation level among each quartile of achievement test performance appeared to lessen the probability that a given activity type might be used as a predictor of academic achievement. A moderately high degree of agreement on activity selection in each of twenty—three schools shows the probability of activity types being common across schools. This may also imply the likelihood of similar findings for comparisons among different neighbor— hoods, economic conditions, and races. The ordering of activity types according to average achievement test scores of participants was compared to the ratings of activity types by teachers for educative value. Teachers' ratings of educational value seemed to have little or no relationship to the rankings of activities by academic test scores. This analysis provides some baseline information which may be useful to the development of school curricula 67 relevant to the everyday experiences of elementary school students. Findings The following findings are reached as a result of this study and its analysis: 1. The activity types in which fifth grade students are most likely to participate on an after-school afternoon or evening include: television viewing, outdoor play, house- hold chores, and schoolwork. Somewhat more specifically, these include: indoor chores, sports, schoolwork, and viewing movies, adventure dramas, or situation comedies on television. 2. The types of activities found to be common among students in all schools, at all levels of achievement, and on any school day were generally, but not always, also the most popular among the total population. 3. The day of the week (Monday through Thursday) has no effect on the types of activities most often occurring throughout the given population of students.’ 4. There is a moderately high level of agreement among schools regarding the relative participation levels of after- school activity types. 5. The pattern of most frequently occurring activities does not appear to vary with the achievement level of the students. 68 6. The ordering of subcategories of activities by the average standardized test scores of participants could raise some speculation for future research, but no definite conclusions are forthcoming from this study. This information may be skewed by the small number of respondents claiming participation in some subcategories. 7. According to teacher ratings the following after— school activities would be most highly recommended as educational and likely to have a positive effect on future learning: visiting the public library, using study reference materials (dictionaries and encyclopedias), visiting a museum, watching the news on television, looking for information in special interest books (sports, languages, science, poems), having science as a hobby, reading the newspaper, reading biographies, and doing schoolwork. 8. There appears to be no relationship between the ranking of after-school activities by the average achievement test scores of participants and the ranking of these same activities according to teacher ratings. Discussion The information summarized in this study was compiled from the input of 1050 fifth grade students in 23 different elementary schools within a single school district during a four day period in late spring. The limitations this places on generalizing the conclusions to other populations or to 69 other times of the year is recognized. However, with these limitations in mind, classroom teachers and researchers may still find ways to gather and apply similar information with smaller groups, to test these findings experimentally, to expand the data collected by surveying other p0pulations, or to develop curriculum ideas which these findings may suggest. The specific examples provided by respondents in this study were too varied and too numerous to list. They were, therefore, summarized under subcategories where they best seemed to fit. There were some difficulties in determining the fit of specific activities mentioned by students into a manageable number of subcategories. These categories were found to be neither all-inclusive nor mutually exclusive of the examples provided by students. Some omitted activities which were mentioned, although not frequently, include fishing and listening to tapes, records or the radio. This infrequent response may be due in part to the absence of an advanced organizer on the survey form which would remind students to mention these activities. Subsequently these activities were not suggested often and therefore no logical subcategories were provided for their inclusion in the study. Other activities were mentioned frequently and in some cases often enough to justify the creation of a subcategory exclusively for the activity itself, such as bike riding. A few activities were difficult to place, for example playing marbles, which was suggested both as an indoor and as an 7O outdoor activity. It was finally decided to place it in the subcategory of sports since marbles was most often mentioned in the survey responses along with other outdoor sport activities. The ability of the students to provide more specific examples of their actual activities as suggested by the categories and subcategories enables the teacher to draw from them topics of interest for purposes of discussion, analysis, and connection to learning objectives. This possibility is enhanced further when the teacher is aware of the probability that students have common experiential background on a given topic. The listings of activity types along with the percent of respondents claiming participation in these activities provides a starting point for relevant topic selection. The long term reliability of the findings of this study deserves more investigation. We might expect some common after-school activities to vary with the time of the year and from year to year. Along the same lines, new technology may result in new cultural trends which affect the activity selections of children. For example, Hurlock, in earlier editions of Adolescent Development indicated that television was becoming increasingly popular, but was still a luxury.113 Subsequent editions have modified that accounting of the prevalence of television, which is now researched as a major 113Elizabeth B. Hurlock, Adolescent Development, 2nd ed. (New York: McGraw—Hill, 1955),]nn 278—280. 71 influence on our youth.114 A similar phenomena indicated in this study and already the subject of research is that of video and computer-type games. Since the relative frequency of occurence of after- school activities of students appears to be common across schools and levels of achievement, we might conclude that they may be universally used as relevant topics for study or as an experiential basis for understanding and applying skills learned in school. This should not suggest ignoring the study of less common topics, but it does permit the teacher to choose the proper mix of the familiar and novel to enhance both the comfort and challenge necessary for efficient learning. On closer investigation of specific activities, how— ever, the apparent common participation in similar activity types among subgroups may not be entirely accurate. The scope of this study was too broad to draw definite conclusions on the implications of specific activities. However, some cursory observations are made here which might be worth further study. At the time of the study Dungeons and Dragons gamebook activities were mentioned only by students at one particular school which houses the district's program for gifted 11"see, for example, Marie Winn, The Plugein Drug; Television, Children and the Family (New York: The Viking Press, 1977). 72 students. This would explain the high ranking of this activity using academic test scores. It does not fully explore why this activity was occurring at a single location among bright students. Could it hold a special challenge or interest attractive to a select group of students? Did a teacher at the school introduce the activity as one of potential interest to the students? These questions and others could also be applied to other anomalies which became evident with the identification of specific activities occurring only at a few schools. The cultural differences appear to be downplayed by the similar comparisons of subcategory rankings among schools. However, more specific activity identification could lead to grounds for distinguishing among cultural groups within schools. For example, jumping rope and "double dutch" (a type of jumping rope with two ropes going simultaneously in opposite directions) were both lumped together as exercise activities. However, "double dutch" was more frequently mentioned in schools with predominantly Black populations. The reasons for the popularity of this activity at certain schools and how it came to be introduced at these schools would be an interesting investigation. Another study which could be undertaken using more specific activities is that of the television viewing of certain shows. Although students listed program titles on the open-ended survey used in this study, the programs of 73 similar types were grouped together as one subcategory. It might be of interest to more closely examine whether situation comedies, for instance, attract higher percentages of Black or Latino audiences, if minority actors are among the primary players in the show. The implications for educators, however, from the studies of specific activities would be found not so much from the statistical differences which may exist among cultural subgroups, but more in the analysis of content of these activities and how they as familiar activities could be used to support new learning. The shifting of focus from specific activities which may be used to differentiate among subcultures to categories which transcend subcultures and provide a basis of commonality implies the ability of educators and learners alike to shift levels of analysis. This is described by Leichter as the concept of resonance which is both a technique for understanding the inter— connections within a culture as well as a means of cutting across heterogeneity within a complex society.115 This concept of resonance may be used, for example, as the explanation for a teacher seeking to identify the common elements of television comedies which make them so popular, by drawing from the students' knowledge of the specific programs they have watched. In more general terms the 115Leichter, Families and Communities as Educators, pp. 27-32 0 74 concept of resonance also provides a rationale and suggests a method for connecting the various out-of—school experiences of the individual learners to school learning guided by the teacher. According to the comparisons used in this study, it appears that there is no obvious inherent learning of skills related to academic achievement occurring merely from participation in an activity. Any learning which is to be derived therefrom must apparently be made more explicit by adult intervention and mediation. Therefore the teacher's opinion and awareness of a child's life experiences deserve more attention in the process of selecting curriculum content. This is especially true if the attempt is made to explain or justify why no relationship was found between the ranking of activities by the average academic test scores of participants and by the teachers' ratings of these same activities for educational value. On the one hand, it would appear that teachers missed the mark, if they perceived their task as identifying those activities which correlate most highly with high academic achievement. Some of the teachers' highest choices such as going to the library, using reference materials, and even doing schoolwork were, in fact, ranked relatively low by the measure of concurrent academic test scores. Although ranking by standardized test scores appears to be a more objective measure than teachers' opinion, it is also more prone to a skewing of the average by extreme scores 75 when an activity has a low number of indicated participants. The comparing of academic test achievement to activity participation does not, of itself, provide any obvious reasons for ranking the activities and assigning educative value accordingly. Still, it would be of high interest and value to look for reasons through closer examination of the relationship between a ranking by academic test score and the variables affecting activity selection or the variables affected by activity participation. On the other hand, it could easily be assumed that teachers have justifiable reasons for their opinions of what are educationally valuable activities. These reasons might not necessarily coincide with the variables that could explain variations in achievement test scores. Educationally valuable life experiences might not be perceived as events directly related to school learning as measured on tests, although the ten t0p ranked activities by teachers such as going to the library, reading various sources for information, and schoolwork do appear to be closely tied to normal school activity. although this study only undertook to investigate degrees of association, future studies might hypothesize cause and effect relationships to see perhaps if the school influence itself has encouraged more participation in certain activities by all students. This could result in a normalization, if not a direct lowering, of the average achievement test scores. 0n the other hand it might also 76 raise some individual scores where prior experience of fewer school related activities would have resulted in lower academic performance. Why, for example, does library attendance rank so low according to average test scores? Examination of library locations throughout the city and programs offered, such as movies, puppet shows, and reading incentive programs, reveals that the libraries in the community studied are reaching a broad spectrum of the population, many of whom live in depressed economic areas where many factors may impinge on academic success. This should not be perceived as a short—‘ coming of the libraries to increase achievement or as a failure of library attenders to raise their levels of learning. On the contrary, it could indicate that library services are being extended to persons who have the most to gain from access to educational resources. The outreach of public schools to encourage activities associated with academic learning might be similarly construed. It is in keeping with the principles of comprehensive and universal education. Nevertheless, evidence of successful outreach should not furnish grounds for complacency. There is also evidence to support the need for a more effective outreach to raise levels of academic learning and the need to include in our definition of comprehensive schooling a recognition in the elementary grades of the potential value of common daily experiences. 77 According to the theories and findings of this study, the promotion of academic activity does not sufficiently meet the conditions leading to academic success. Evidence of continuing motivation, deepening of interest and feelings of competence are also conditions of effective learning within a given experience. Teachers might use these theories and findings to broaden the scope of their own opinion of educational value by discovering how after—school activities, which are either popular among many students or hold the prolonged interest of a few, can be used as the bridges between schooling and life experiences which make learning relevant. The influence of a teacher is important. This influence is enhanced when the teacher enables the learner to make connections to his, the learner's, own personal experience. The findings of this study do not support the valuing of any single activity as more educational than another. Neither does it appear appropriate to conclude that one valuing system, whether based upon level of participation, association with test achievement, or professional opinion, is superior to another which yields different results. What is suggested is that activities have potential educative value through their association with growth into new experiences. Participation in any or all activities is an interactive experience of both discovering and demonstrating competence. Within the school, curriculum can be developed 78 which blends familiar and novel experiences through an associative learning process. The educative value of after- school activities, therefore, is not to be regarded as an inherent quality or quantity found within experiences, but rather an activity attains value as it is mediated by adults and connected by the child to further experience which facilitates the growth of the learner. Conclusion The overall conclusion of this study, is that the search for factors which link out—of—school activity participation to academic achievement is not satisfied by a comparison of teachers' ratings, rankings by average test scores, and levels of participation. While separately they provide data of interest, these various systems of ranking the after—school activities of fifth graders do not appear consistent with each other nor do they indicate which factors within common daily activities influence academic achievement or which distinguish successful learners from unsuccessful learners. This leads to the conclusion that researchers might better look for more explicit forms of identifying how a child's out-of—school and in—school experiences can be connected. It is suggested that teachers should have a knowledge of the activities mentioned by students in this study and an awareness of the implied relationships to participation level, other teachers' opinions, and academic 79 achievement test scores. With this understanding these teachers can mediate learning from the experiences a learner already has and can influence the experiences a learner will have that affect his growth. The following organization and elaboration of this conclusion provides some examples of its implementation as an educational practice: 1. Using the most common experiences. Teachers could incorporate in their lesson planning the common out—of—school experiences of their students by referring to the levels of participation for activities listed in this study. For example, television watching is an activity common to almost all children. Teachers could connect television viewing to reading objectives by examining the storylines of television adventure dramas to illustrate concepts such as plot, setting, climax, characters, author's purpose and elements of interest. They may be able to obtain transcripts of television shows and examine them by reading of the dialogue in a program. Knowing that students frequently go to stores or restaurants, teachers might inquire further to learn specifically where students go and what they or their parents buy. This could serve as an introduction to concepts in elementary economics or as a basis of true-to-life math story problems. The result of this integration of common experience with learning might be a reduction in the disconnectedness or even dissonance which occurs between school lessons and out-of—school experience. 80 Bridging the gap in this manner, the teacher uses what is familiar to the student to introduce new learning, provide challenge at an appropriate level and create the conditions necessary for efficient learning. The learner can, as a result, see relevance in his learning and will be more likely to retain information and transfer this learning to new situations and personal growth. 2. Using the experiences associated with high academic achievement test scores. Some activities are associated with high achievement test scores, but if there are factors which connect them to high achievement, these factors are not immediately apparent. According to the comparisons used in this study, it appears that there is no learning of skills related to academic achievement occurring merely from participation in an out—of— school activity. The importance of other factors which can be more easily observed, need to be considered. An example of observable connection—making is the mediation of an activity by significant others such as teachers, parents or friends. In the school setting, this specifically suggests that the teacher can play the important role of interpreter and mediator of activities as educative experiences. For instance, the teacher can suggest to students that they discuss an activity such as caring for pets, since participants in this activity had a high average of 81 achievement test scores. The teacher might then ask the students to suggest what aspects of caring for a pet require thought and learning. The teacher might also instruct the students that reading for information can be specific to an interest such as a pet, that caring for another such as a pet is similar to caring about ourselves and other people, or that the same intense interest felt in playing with a pet can be felt when subject matter catches one's interest. These and other analogous connections from students and teachers permit learners to reflect upon relationships made explicit‘ by the discussion. Learning, as viewed from this perspective, occurs not so much at the point of participation in an activity as from the learner's reflection on his experience before and after the event. By stimulating, encouraging, and even directing reflection, teachers should be able to increase the likelihood that the competencies demonstrated within an activity can be associated with the learning of skills related to academic achievement. 3. Using the activities rated highly educational by teachers. Those activities in the study which one would expect to be related to school learning and which were, in addition, highly rated by teachers were, nevertheless, not found to be associated with high achievement test scores. Therefore the conclusion is reached that the promotion of academic type 82 activities alone does not sufficiently meet the conditions leading to academic success. Again it is suggested that other factors such as mediation and motivation be considered. Teachers have the opportunity and ought to have the training and experience which enables them to monitor the interests and competencies of their students in activities which they, the educators, consider important. The connections which they feel are important and the results which they would like to see from participation in a valued activity can be made explicit through direct communication of ideas from the teachers and by encouraging the students to express what they feel they are learning in a given situation. For example, the opportunities available at the local public library, rated highly educational by teachers, can be considered from the experience of students as well as the experience of teachers. Together they could analyze whether students and teachers agree on what attracts people to visit the library and how it can be used. As a creative endeavor students might brainstorm ideas of what they would like to see available for borrowing from the library and how such books or items would assist their learning about important topics. All of this suggests that while teachers hope that students learn implicitly from the activities teachers associate with academic learning, the teacher's ability to monitor whether such learning is occurring is increased by making explicit their expectations relevant to the activity. 83 4. Using activities mentioned but not highly ranked in any grouping. The conclusion of this study does not support the valuing of any single activity nor any particular ranking system as a predictor of academic success or as a measure of educational worth. On the contrary, it suggests that nearly every activity holds the potential to become an educative experience if the teacher mediating the experience is able to perceive the competencies which the student is demonstrating and accurately predict the next direction of growth which seems appropriate for the learner. For example, the teacher may decide to focus on a topic of low participation level and one which is not otherwise associated with high test achievement or high teacher rating such as building models. Through instruction and discussion, the teacher may interest more students in building a model, inform students of direction following teachniques which make model building easier, or reinforce feelings of competence among students who successfully complete construction of a model. This is to illustrate that the educative value of activity may be found more clearly in the processes influencing activity than in the activity itself. However, more research is needed to investigate how growth into new experience comes about from instruction and reflection on familiar experience. 84 Implications for future research Stephen Hamilton suggests a four—tier hierarchy for evaluation of experiential learning programs: 1. participant responses 2. measurementinstruments 3. attribution of measurable effects to a particular program . attributipg of particular effects to various properties of a program In other words, 1. Do participants say they have been affected? 2. Is there evidence of effects? 3. Is there evidence that the program was responsible for the effects? 4. What phout the program was responsible for the effects?1 It was presumed in this study that inquiry into the potential of learning from common prior experience may be considered evaluation of a type of experiential learning program. This seems to be supported by Hamilton who states: People learn without programs, and the efforts should be made to determine what sorts of things are learned "incidentally" and how that learning occurs. Research on the conditions under which development occurs would be most useful to the designers of experiential learning programs and their evaluators.118 116Stephan Hamilton, "Evaluating Experiential Learning Programs," paper presented at the 63rd annual meeting of the American Educational Research Association, San Francisco, 8- 12 April 1979, pp.3—6. 117Ibid., p. 4. 118Ibid., p. 21. 85 This research focused primarily on the first level of evaluation, that of participant responses, with preliminary attempts to extend the base information gathered to higher levels by comparing students responses to the measuring instruments of a standardized achievement test and then looking for the possibility of relating achievement differences to particular activities. Further research at all levels might be suggested. This study attempted to gather in an open-ended fashion a broad base of information from participants regarding their activities after the normal school day was completed. Consequently, the quality and even the quantity of information received from individual respondents on different days was not rigidly controlled. Using this knowledge of the variability of responses collected on an open-ended survey, the following recommendations to control the gathering of information from participants on this topic in the future are offered: 1. Limit the choice of responses to a checklist of specific activities which appeared in the initial survey to be most popular, thereby increasing the likelihood of a higher, yet controlled response. 2. Select for another survey only those specific activities, which by reasons suggested in the literature or as a result of questions raised by the broad survey, appear to hold a particular common attribute which may in turn 86 effect some aspect of learning. 3. If the information is desired to assist in determining an educational plan for a particular group such as a classroom or school, limit the population surveyed to the target group. 4. Collect more precisely information on other variables which may be significant to research, such as, sex differences, racial or ethnic differences, economic differences, or cultural patterns. 5. Compare the findings for the population studied in this survey to other pOpulations, such as in other grade levels or in other cities. 6. Compare the findings for the season of the year studied in this research to other seasons in the school year. Further research might also consider improving upon or varying the use of measuring instruments. In this study comparisons were made using the California Achievement Test and a survey of teacher opinion regarding the educative value of activities. The literature suggests outcomes other than just academic achievement from experiential learning. These include, for example, career skills, feelings of competence, and more responsible attitudes. The following suggestions might be considered for future research: 1. Compare activity participation to measures of affective behavior such as self—concept. 2 Compare activity participation to another measure of 87 academic achievement such as grade point average. 3. Develop an instrument to measure teachers' knowledge or experience as well as opinions of children's common activities. 4. Compare subtests of specific skills to specific activities which seem to require the skills being measured by the tests. Although the analysis undertaken to summarize this study did not reveal any strong attribution of measurable effect to a particular activity, a more controlled experiment might still explore this possibility. Along these lines research of the following types might be attempted: 1. Create the opportunity for an experimental group over a period of time to participate more actively in a selected common experience such as playing a particular type of game, viewing a particular television show, or assisting at home with a particular chore. Compare the expected learning outcomes of the experimental group to a control group. 2. To study the influence of school learning on continuing motivation, instruct and encourage an experimental group in the application of a skill to a common out-of—school activity. Observe if this instruction increases the interest and the participation in the activity by the experimental group over that of a control group. Finally, although not attempted in this research, it 88 should be of interest to study the attribution of particular effects to the various properties of a common activity. Again an investigation of this type might best be designed as an experiment to explore some of the following questions: 1. Does, as the literature suggests, the intervention of adult mediation encourage reflection on a common activity and enhance the learning derived from it? 2. If a skill inherent to a common activity is pointed out and practiced, will the activity performance be improved? Will there be a carryover to related learning? 3. Can discussion of common experience be incorporated into the instructional setting with measurable improvement in 'academic learning? 4. Can the independent practice of an academic skill be integrated into a common after-school activity with measurable improvement in learning? 5. How might maturity levels be indicated by activity? Do these indicators correlate positively with other measures of maturity? 6. How might selection of after-school activities be influenced by a teacher? The data collected in this research is preliminary information in the formation of hypotheses for future research. It provides a range of tOpics identified as common activities of fifth graders. It suggests probable participation levels in these activities for the population 89 studied. It summarizes the average academic test scores of participants in identified activities. It provides ratings of activity categories and subcategories from the viewpoint of teachers. A major test of the significance of these findings would come with the replication of this type of information gathering and organization with similar populations to see if the relative popularity of activities is the same, to see if the ordering of activity type by average academic test performance is the same, and to see if the teachers' ratings of activities as educative are the same. More information gathering and comparisons of this type would shed further light on the question of whether the educative value of an activity is inherent in experience or whether it is brought about primarily as an outcome of adult mediation. APPENDIX A SURVEY FORMS AND LETTERS 90 Dear With permission from the Testing and Evaluation Department of the Saginaw Board of Education, I am conducting research on the relationship between a child's out of school activities and his in sdhool achievement. I need your assistance first of all in discovering what the common out of school activities of child- ren are in such areas as television viewing, household chores. going on trips. games, sports. and hobbies. As a followhup to . the gathering of this data. I would like to share the information with you and ask.your opinion of the educational value of the specific activities which children list. Accompanying this cover letter is a set of questionnaires for students, asking them to list for four days some of their out of school activities under the categories suggested. Please give the students the survey for Monday's activities on Tuesday, and so on, so that they list the previous day's activities on the following morning and complete a list of after school activities for Monday through Thursday by Friday of the week surveyed. Thank you for your assistance. This research is being done in partial fulfillment of my doctoral degree requirements throuthMichigan State University. I will be happy to share with you a.summery of my findings upon completion of the study. Sincerely, Maw/0.2%.}: Charles A. Shelley. Jr. 91 even Hm m mcnGNQOWHmm mXWBNWWM H. Sonar H< N. U0 SOCmmSOHQ Croemm w. meK omamm Hbaoonm b. pHmK ocomwmm mooH 00000 o 000 0000 000 owOuld not recommend I—‘I-‘I-‘l-‘H H |-'I-*|-‘ I-‘I-W-‘H I-‘t-‘P HWould recommend as enter- wwwww (.0 www wwww www wWould recommend as educational bbbbb e not: oeee poo eWOUJ-d recommend as mtjrim namroaam Page 2 '94 H to u .s .2 H-u-l 3 an.) (D on E :0 0 0:3 to 'O 'O o no a) m 0) mm as on ‘ H on: 'O 'o go I: c: c 0:: mu m o 00 EC 5 H-r-l c: r-i +3 O on O 4.3a: U~ 0c: 0 00 (Du mo (D :23 Ht: H'H H '08 “Om '03 "O .-1 FIE n40 .-| a: a; as a 3:: 344 3 434343 $543 hhbJ-‘nh-P- 45 ab 43.0 highly educational 95 VI. Taking lessons com» Music (instrument or voice) Dancing or gymnastics Self defense (judo, karate) Interests (sewing, gardening, ice skating) VII. Going to the public library VIII.Going interesting places A. B. C. D. E. F. G. H. I. J. Stores and restaurants Movies Plays and concerts Zoos and nature parks Parks and playgrounds Hospital to visit someone Friend or relatives house Nearby cities and towns Museum Sporting event IX. Reading for fun QWMUOU’J’ Stories (library books) Comic books Newspaper Magazines Romances Special interests (sports, science Biographies (people in sports & history) Would not recommend not educational 0 0 O O O O O 0 O 0 O O O O O O O O 0 O O O O O O 0 not as educational Would recommend as enter- tainment, H HHHHHHH HHHHHHHHHHH H H HHHH H WOuld recommend as somewhat educational NNNNNNN NNNNNNNNNNN N N NNNN N N uowtuua ‘» Would recommend as educational wwwwwww wwwwwwwwwww (.0 U) 0») Page 3 highly educational A eoooeee eeeeeeeeeee a 4:. oboe 4s W0111d recomend as X. Looking in a book for information A. B. C. - uages, science, poems, Bible) XI. Going to a meeting A. B. C. D. XII. Working or a hobby or project A. B. C. D. B. F. G. H. I. 96 Would not recommend not educational Study-reference (dictionary encyclopedia, textbook) Household reference (telephone book, TV guide, catalog) Special interest (Sports, lang- Scouts or 4-H Church group Neighborhood club or gang Choir or play rehearsal Making models Collections (stamp, coins, rocks) Painting and Drawing Crafts Woodwork Gardening Needlework (sewing, knitting,etc.) Science experiments Home or automobile repairs 0000000000 00000 O O O O 0 Would recommend as enter- Page 4 F... «:4: 52 ".43 1:32 00 on 'O (DU) m m «I'D c: 461(1) e5 0 ~O #0) CH O E o ,4 «43 «so ,u 3 l 2 l 2 l 2 l 2 l 2' l 2 l 2 l 2 l 2 l 2 1 . 2 l 2 l 2 l 2 l 2 l 2 l 2 l 2 l 2 l 2 educational 1» a; I» Would recommend as educational mu wwuww uuuwwwwwww Would recommend as highly educational .aoeeeeoeoe apnea e e e e o APPENDIX B TABLES 97 TABLE 4.1 RANKING OF ACTIVITIES BY PARTICIPATION LEVEL Number % of Activity Rank Selecting Population Chores Indoors 1 883 84.10 Sports 2 848 80.76 Schoolwork 3 832 79.24 Tv Movie or Drama 4 804 76.57 Sitcom 5 799 76.09 Stories 6 506 48.14 Cartoons 7 388 36.95 Store or Restaurant 8 352 33.52 Exercise 9 335 31.90 Bike Riding 10 333 31.71 Study Reference 11 302 28.76 Chance Board Games 12 268 25.52 Video Games 13.5 253 24.10 Friends or Relatives 13.5 253 24.10 Outside Make Believe 15 213 20.29 Library 16 201 19.14 Soap Opera 17 167 15.90 Inside Make Believe 18 154 14.67 Parks and Playgrounds 19 148 14.10 Chores Outdoors 20 145 13.81 Special Interests 21 138 13.14 Scouts 22 133 12.67 Cards 23 131 12.48 TV News 24 130 12.38 Comics 25 127 12.10 Skill Board Games 26 119 11.13 Music 27 117 11.14 Models 28 106 10.10 Household Reference 29 98 9.33 Draw 30 93 8.86 Pets 31 91 8.67 Sporting Event 32.5 82 7.81 Repairs 32.5 82 7.81 Pool, Ping Pong 34 78 7.43 Swimming 35.5 74 7.05 TV Sports 35.5 74 7.05 Church 37 81 6.76 Game Shows 38 67 6.38 Dance or Gymnastics 39 65 6.19 Variety Show 40.5 64 6.10 98 TABLE 4.1 Continued Number %;of Activity Rank Selecting Population Nearby City 40.5 64 6.10 Collections 42 62 5.91 Newspaper 43 56 5.33 Interest Books 44 55 5.24 Magazines 45.5 53 5.05 Crafts 45.5 53 5.05 Hiking 47 48 4.57 Needlework 48 46 4.38 Cooking 49.5 43 4.10 Movies 49.5 43 4.10 Club 51 41 3.90 Woodwork 52.5 40 3.81 Self Defense 52.5 40 3.81 Gardening 54 38 3.62 Science 55 33 3.14 Babysitting 56 29 2.76 Hospital or Doctor 57 27 2.57 Puzzles 58 24 2.29 Interest Lessons 59.5 22 2.10 Family Drama 59.5 22 2.10 Rehearsal 61 21 2.00 Plays and Concerts 62 17 1.62 Biographies 63 13 1.24 Dungeons and Dragons 64 10 .96 Zoo 65 9 .86 Museum 66 8 .76 Romances 67 4 .38 99 TABLE 4.4 RANKING 0F ACTIVITIES BY TOTAL CAT BATTERY SCORES CAT % of Activity Rank Average Population Dungeons and Dragons 1 188.70 .96 Romances 2 175.50 .38 Gardening 3 171.05 3.62 Collections 4 170.53 5.91 Pets 5 170.09 8.67 Variety Show 6 169.80 6.10 Science 7 168.00 3.14 Interest Books 8 167.07 5.24 Babysitting 9 164.66 2.76 Museum 10 164.38 .76 Hiking 11 163.85 4.57 Magazines 12 163.70 5.05 Puzzles 13 163.25 2.29 Bike Riding 14 162.52 31.71 Sporting Event 15 161.65 7.81 Soap Opera 16 161.28 15.90 TV News 17 161.19 12.38 Game Shows 18 160.15 6.38 Newspaper 19 160.09 5.33 Crafts 20 160.04 5.05 Hospital or Doctor 21 159.26 2.57 Interest Lessons 22 159.09 2.10 Outside Make Believe 23 158.69 20.29 Video Games 24 158.16 24.10 Swimming 25 157.84 7.05 Dance or Gymnastics 26 157.48 6.19 Rehearsal 27 157.05 2.00 Cooking 28 156.79 4.10 Nearby City 29 155.83 6.10 Scouts 30 155.81 12.67 Store or Restaurant 31 155.41 33.52 Plays and Concerts 32 155.29 1.62 Comics 33 155.13 12.10 Stories 34 154.70 48.19 TV Movie or Drama 35 154.38 76.57 Sitcom 36 154.03 76.09 Pool, Ping Pong 37 153.58 7.43 Biographies 38 153.38 1.24 Friends or Relatives 39 153.00 24.10 100 TABLE 4.4 Continued L CAT % of Activity Rank Average Population Study Reference 40 152.99 28.76 Household Reference 41 152.96 9.33 Draw 42 152.86 8.86 Music 43 152.81 11.14 Cartoons 44.5 152.57 36.96 Schoolwork 44.5 152.57 79.24 Needlework 46 152.41 4.38 Sports 47 152.27 80.76 Special Interests 48 152.25 13.14 Skill Board Games 49 152.15 11.33 Chores Outdoors 50 152.04 13.81 Chores Indoors 51 151.99 84.10 Cards 52 151.24 12.48 TV Sports 53 150.73 7.05 Exercise 54 149.84 31.90 Zoo 55 149.67 .86 Chance Board Games 56 148.06 25.52 Models 57 146.70 10.10 Church 58 146.18 6.76 Woodwork 59 145.66 3.81 Parks and Playgrounds 60 145.43 14.10 Inside Make Believe 61 145.42 14.67 Club 62 145.24 3.90 Library 63 142.35 19.14 Movies 64 138.26 4.10 Repairs 65 133.93 7.81 Family Drama 66 133.64 2.10 Self Defense 67 122.28 3.81 101 TABLE 4.5 RANKING OF ACTIVITIES BY READING SUBTEST SCORES Average % of Activity Rank Score Population Dungeons and Dragons 1 59.40 .96 Romances 2 51.25 .38 Science 3 50.39 3.14 Pets 4 50.37 8.67 Variety Show 5 49.98 6.10 Collections 6 49.77 5.91 Gardening 7 49.34 3.62 Interest Books 8 49.31 5.24 Puzzles 9 49.25 2.29 Magazines 10 49.04 5.05 Hiking 11 48.69 4.57 Crafts 12 47.96 5.05 Interest Lessons 13 47.77 2.10 Bike Riding 14 47.71 31.71 TV News 15 47.53 12.38 Babysitting 16 47.34 2.76 Zoo 17 47.33 .86 Museum 18 47.25 .76 Outside Make Believe 19 46.89 20.29 Nearby City 20 46.78 6.10 Rehearsal 21 46.67 2.00 Sporting Event 22 46.65 7.81 Comics 23 46.24 12.10 Soap Opera 24 46.16 15.90 Hospital or Doctor 25 45.85 2.57 Newspaper 26 45.77 5.33 Video Games 27.5 45.62 24.10 Biographies 27.5 45.62 1.24 Game Shows 29 45.57 6.38 Swimming 30 45.55 7.05 Scouts 31 45.53 12.67 Dance or Gymnastics 32 45.31 6.19 Store or Restaurant 33 45.13 33.52 Stories 34 45.03 48.19 Study Reference 35 45.00 28.76 Sitcom 36 44.86 76.09 TV Movie or Drama 37 44.82 76.57 Cooking 38 44.81 4.10 Club 39 44.56 3.90 102 TABLE 4.5 Continued Average % of Activity Rank Score Population Music 40 44.52 11.14 Models 41 44.51 10.10 Chores Outdoors 42 44.50 13.81 Cartoons 43 44.47 36.95 Special Interests 44 44.32 13.14 Needlework 45 44.28 4.38 Schoolwork 46 44.15 79.24 Sports 47.5 43.92 80.76 Chores Indoors 47.5 43.92 84.10 Draw 49 43.57 8.86 Friends or Relatives 50 43.53 24.10 Skill Board Games 51 43.52 11.33 Cards 52 43.47 12.48 Pool, Ping Pong 53 43.26 7.43 Exercise 54 43.14 31.90 Plays and Concerts 55 43.12 1.62 Household Reference 56 42.76 9.33 TV Sports 57 42.73 7.05 Chance Board Games 58 42.63 25.52 Inside Make Believe 59 42.38 14.67 Church 60 42.34 6.76 Parks and Playgrounds 61 42.09 14.10 Library 62 41.05 19.14 Movies 63 40.91 4.10 Woodwork 64 40.60 3.81 Repairs 65 39.50 7.81 Family Drama 66 37.91 2.10 Self Defense 67 35.68 3.81 103 TABLE 4.6 RANKING OF ACTIVITIES BY MATH TEST SCORE Average f% of Activity Rank Score Population Dungeons and Dragons 1 64.10 .96 Romances 2 63.50 .38 Pets 3 60.63 8.67 Collections 4 60.52 5.91 Gardening 5 59.11 3.62 Variety show 6 58.47 6.10 Game shows 7 58.12 6.38 Interest books 8.5 58.00 5.24 Museum 8.5 58.00 .76 Sporting event 10 56.99 7.81 Soap opera 11 56.93 15.90 Newspaper 12 56.82 5.33 Hiking 13 56.75 4.57 Science 14 56.51 3.14 Babysitting 15 56.34 2.76 Swimming 16 56.20 7.05 Bike riding 17 56.11 31.71 TV news 18 56.04 12.38 Video games 19 55.83 24.10 Magazines 20 55.15 5.05 Plays and concerts 21 55.12 1.62 Outside make believe 22 55.08 20.29 Hospital or doctor 23 55.00 2.57 Pool ping pong 24 54.91 7.43 Crafts 25 54.83 5.05 Interest lessons 26 54.82 2.10 Dance or gymnastics 27 54.60 6.19 Friends or relatives 28 54.59 24.10 Store or restaurant 29 54.53 33.52 TV sports 30 54.41 7.05 Household reference 31 54.40 9.33 Puzzles 32 54.33 2.29 Cooking 33 54.26 4.10 Scouts 34 54.24 12.67 TV movie or drama 35 54.11 76.57 Comics 36 54.06 12.10 Skill board games 37 53.83 11.33 Stories 38 53.60 48.19 Draw 39 53.57 8.86 Sitcom 40 53.53 76.09 104 TABLE 4.6 Continued Average % of Activity Rank Score Population Cards 41 53.44 12.48 Needlework 42.5 53.41 4.38 Sports 42.5 53.41 80.76 Music 44 53.39 11.14 Schoolwork 45 53.32 79.24 Chores indoors 46 53.07 84.10 Biographies 47 52.85 1.24 Cartoons 48.5 52.76 36.95 Chores outdoors 48.5 52.76 13.81 Rehearsal 50 52.57 2.00 Woodwork 51 52.50 3.81 Special interests 52 52.45 13.14 Study reference 53 52.38 28.76 Nearby city 54 52.03 6.10 Exercise 55 51.92 31.90 Chance board games 56 51.57 25.52 Parks and playgrounds 57 50.86 14.10 Church 58 50.38 6.76 Inside make believe 59 50.08 14.67 Models 60 49.91 10.10 Library 61 49.58 19.14 Club 62 49.12 3.90 Zoo 63 48.22 .86 Family drama 64 46.91 2.10 Movies 65 46.72 4.10 Repairs 66 45.54 7.81 Self defense 67 42.65 3.81 105 TABLE 4.7 TEACHERS' RATINGS 0F ACTIVITIES Average % of Activity Rank Rating Population Library 1 3.73 19.14 Study reference 2 3.71 28.76 Museum 3 3.64 .76 TV news 4 3.62 12.38 Interest books 5 3.59 5.24 Science 6 3.57 3.14 Newspaper 7.5 3.54 5.33 Biographies 7.5 3.54 1.24 Schoolwork 9 3.41 79.24 Music 10 3.39 11.14 Stories 11 3.31 48.19 Repairs 12.5 3.29 7.81 Zoo 12.5 3.29 .86 Plays and concerts 14 3.28 1.62 Special interests 15 3.23 13.14 Collections 16 3.14 5.91 Household reference 17.5 3.12 9.33 Interest lessons 17.5 3.12 2.10 Scouts 19 3.11 12.67 Draw 20.5 3.06 8.86 Magazines 20.5 3.06 5.05 Models 23 3.03 10.10 Pets 23 3.03 8.67 Hiking 23 3.03 4.57 Church 26.5 2.97 6.76 Crafts 26.5 2.97 5.05 Woodwork 26.5 2.97 3.81 Rehearsal 26.5 2.97 2.00 Gardening 29 2.94 3.62 Needlework 30 2.91 4.38 Cooking 31 2.89 4.10 Dance or gymnastics 32 2.86 6.19 Skill board games 33 2.83 11.33 Family drama 34 2.82 2.10 Nearby city 35.5 2.78 6.10 Self defense 35.5 2.78 3.81 Sports 37.5 2.75 80.76 Babysitting 37.5 2.75 2.76 Inside make believe 39 2.65 14.67 Chores outdoors 40 2.61 13.81 Exercise 41 2.58 31.90 106 Table 4.7 Continued Average % of Activity Rank Rating POpulation Chores indoors 42 2.57 84.10 Hospital or doctor 43 2.56 2.57 Game shows 44 2.49 6.38 Friends or relatives 45 2.43 24.10 Swimming 46 2.40 7.05 Parks and playgounds 47 2.31 14.10 Puzzles 48 2.23 2.29 Bike riding 49.5 2.22 31.71 Comics 49.5 2.22 12.10 Sporting event 51 2.19 7.81 Store or restaurant 52 2.17 33.52 Chance board games 53 2.12 25.52 Cards 54 2.11 12.48 Movies 55.5 2.06 4.10 Dungeons and Dragons 55.5 2.06 .96 Outside make believe 57.5 1.94 20.29 Club 57.5 1.94 3.90 TV sports 59 1.92 7.05 TV movie or drama 60 1.85 76.57 Video games 61 1.77 24.10 Pool ping pong 62 1.76 7.43 Variety show 63 1.63 6.10 Sitcom 64 1.31 76.09 Romances 65 1.21 .38 Soap opera 66 .83 15.90 Cartoons 67 .66 36.95 BIBLIOGRAPHY BIBLIOGRAPHY Almy, Millie. Ways of Studying Children. New York: Teachers College Press, 1959. Anderson, John FL "The Relation of Attitude to Adjustmentf' Education 73 (December 1952): 210-218. Bernstein, B. Classy_Codes and Control, vol. 1. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul, 1971. Boynton, Paul L. "The Relationship Between Children's Tested Intelligence and Their Hobby ParticipationsJ' Journal of Genetic Psychology 58 (June 1941): 353— 362. Buros, Oscar Krisen. The Eighth Mental Measurements Yearbook, vol. 2. Highland Park, New Jersey: The Gryphon Press, 1978. Bronfenbrenner, Urie. "Who Needs Parent Education?" In Families and Communities as Educators, pp. 203-223. Edited by Hope Jensen Leichter. New York: Teachers College, Columbia University, 1979. Chickering, Arthur W. "Developmental Change As a Major Outcomeu" In Experiential Learning: Rationale, Characteristics and Assessment, pp. 62-107. Edited by Morris T. Keeton. San Francisco: Jossey—Bass, 1976. . Experience and Learning: An Introduction to Experiential Learnipg. New Rochelle, lLY.: Change Magazine Press, 1977. Coleman, James S. "Differences Between Experiential and Classroom Learning." In Experiential Learning; Rationale, Characteristics and Assessment, pp. 49—61. Edited by Morris T. Keeton. San Francisco: Jossey— Bass, 1976. Conrad, Dan, and Hedin, Diane. "Are Experiential Learning Programs Effective?" NASSP Bulletin 62 (November 1978): 102-107. 107 108 . Executive Summary of the Final Report of the Experiential Education Evaluation Project. St. Paul, Minnesota: Center for Youth Development and Research, University of Minnesota. Dave, Ravindrakumar H. "The Identification and Measurement of Environmental Process Variables That Are Related to Educational AchievementJ' PhJL dissertation, University of Chicago, 1963. Davis, Robert H.; Duley, John S.; and Alexander, Lawrence T. Field Experience. East Lansing, Michigan: Michigan State University, 1977. Deci, E. L. Intrinsic Motivation. New York: Plenum Press, 1975. Dewey, John. Democracy and Education. New York: MacMillan, 1916. . Experience and Education. New York: MacMillan, 1938. . The School and Sociepy. Chicago: University of Chicago, 1900. Eckstrom, Ruth B.; Harris, Abigail M.; and Lockheed, Marlaine E. How to Get College Credit for What You Have Learned as a Homemaker and Volunteer. Princeton, New Jersey: Educational Testing Service, 1977. Elkind, David. "How Grown-Ups Help Children LearnJ' Principal 60 (May 1981): 20—24. Ewart, Alan. "The Effects of Outdoor Adventure Activities Upon Self-Concept." Master's Thesis, Eastern Michigan University, 1977. Fletcher, Sara Ann. "A Comparison of Affective Changes Between Economically Disadvantaged and Advantaged Sixth Graders as a Resident Outdoor Education Program." Doctor of Recreation Dissertation, Indiana University, 1973. Foley, Matthew, and McGuire, Donald. "Cognitive Skills and Street Activityu" Urban Education 16 (April 1981): 13-36. 109 Forrest, Aubrey. Assessing Prior Learning: A CAEL Student Guide. Columbia, Maryland: Cooperative Assessment of Experiential Learning, 1977. Freire, Paulo. Education for Critical Consciousness. New York: Seabury Press, 1973. . Pedagogy of the Oppressed. New York: Herder and Herder, 1968. Gaston, Debra Wickstrom; Prouffe, Mary Mahoney; and Chinsky, Jack M. "An Empirical Investigation of a Wilderness School." Paper presented at the 49th Annual Meeting of the Eastern Psychological Association, Washington, D.C" .31 March 1978. Gateway High School: "School Without Walls". New Orleans: Orleans Parish School Board, 1971. Gesell, Arnold, and Ilg, Frances L. The Child from Five to Ten. New York: Harper and Brothers, 1946. Gesell, Arnold; Ilg, Frances L.; and Ames, Louise Bates. Youth: The Years from Ten to Sixteen. New York: Harper and Row, 1956, Getzels, J} W} "The Communities of Education)‘ In.Families and Communities and Educators, pp. 95—118. Edited by Hope Jensen Leichter. New York: Teachers College, Columbia University, 1979. . "Socialization and Education: A Note on Discontinuitiesfl Teachers College Record 76 (December 1974): 218-225. Godfrey, Robert. "A Review of Research and Evaluation Literature on Outward Bound and Related Educational ProgramsJ' IPaper presented at the Conference of Experiential Education, Estes Park, Colorado, 8-11 October 1974. Gordon, Sheila C. "Campus and Workplace as Arenas." In Experiential Learning: Rationale, Characteristics, and Assessment, pp. 108—118. Edited by Morris T. Keeton. San Francisco: Jossey—Bass, 1976. Gordon, William J. J., and Poze, Tony. "Learning Dysfunction and Connection Making” Psychiatric Annals 8 (March 1978): 79—88. 110 Hamilton, Stephen. "Evaluating Experiential Learning Programs." Paper presented at the 63rd Annual Meeting of the American Educational Research Association, San Francisco, 8-12 April 1979. Harter, Susan. "Effectance Motivation Reconsidered." Human .Develqpment 21 (1978): 58. Hurlock, Elizabeth B. Adolescent Develqpment. 4th ed. New York: McGraw—Hill, 1973. . Adolescent Development. 2nd ed. New York: McGraw—Hill, 1955. Hutchkins, Robert C. "School Options in Philadelphia, Their Present and Futurefl Educational Leadership 32 (November 1974): 88—91. Jersild, Arthur T. In Search of Self. New York: Teachers College Press, 1952. Jersild, Arthur T., and Tasch, R. J. Children's Interests and What They Suggest for Education. New York: Teachers College, Columbia University, 1949. Keeton, Morris T. Experiential Learning: Rationaley Characteristicspyand Assessment. San Francisco: Jossey—Bass, 1976. Keeves, J. P. "The Home, the School, and Achievement in Mathematics? Science Education 59 (October 1975): 439—460. Knapp, Joan, and Amiel, Sharon. A Compendium of Assessment Techniques. Columbia, Maryland: CAEL, 1975. Kolb, David A., and Fry, Ronald. "Towards an Applied Theory of Experiential Learningfl In Theories of Groqp Processes, pp. 33—57. Edited by Gary Cooper. New York: John Wiley and Sons, 1975. Lee, William B. "Ecole Moderne Pedagogie Freinet: Educational Bonesetters." Phi Delta Kappan 61 (January 1980): 341—345. Lehman, H. C., and Witty, P. A. The Peychology_of Play Activities. New York: A. S. Barnes and Company, 1927. Leichter, Hope Jensen. Families and Communities as Educators. New York: Teachers College, Columbia University, 1979. 111 . "Some Perspectives of the Family as Educatorf' Teachers College Record 76 (December 1974): 175—217. Lightfoot, Sara Lawrence. Worlds Apart: Relationships Between Families and Schools. New York: Basic Books, 1978. Litwak, Eugene, and Meyer, Henry J. SchoolLyFamily, and Neighborhood; The Theory and Practice of School- Community Relations. New York: Columbia University Press, 1974. Locigno, Michael J. "The Home—life as Curricular PhenomenaJ' Masters thesis, University of Illinois at Chicago Circle, 1979-1980. Marjoribanks, Kevin. Families and Their Learning Environments: An Empirical Analysis. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul, 1979. McLeod, Carmen E. Handbook for Career Specialization Programs. Philadelphia, PA: Research for Better Schools, Inc., 1978. Maehr, Martin Ln "Continuing Motivation: An Analysis of a Seldom Considered Educational Outcomeu" Review of Educational Research 46 (Fall 1976): 443—462. Nias, Davis K. B. "The Classification and Correlates of Children's Academic and Recreational InterestsJ' Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 20 (January 1979): 73—79. Owens, Thomas R.; Haenn, Joseph F.; and Fehrenbacher, Harry L. "The Use of Multiple Strategies in Evaluating an Experienced-Based Career Education Program." Journal of Research and Development in Education 12 (Spring 1979): 35—49. Pressey, S. L., and Kuhlen, R. G. Psychological Development Through the Life Span. New York: Harper and Row, 1957. "Prior Learning: A Guide to Portfolio DevelopmentJ' Columbia, Missouri: Stephens College Without Walls, 1977. The Saginaw Elementary School ProfileLyl981-1982. Saginaw, Michigan: Department of Evaluation Services, Saginaw Public Schools. 112 Schubert, William H. "Knowledge About Out—of—School Curriculum." The Educational Forum 45 (January 1981): 185-198. Staley, Frederick A. "The Research, Evaluation, and Measurement Dragons in Outdoor Education." Paper presented at the National Outdoor Education Association Meeting, Lake Placid, N.Y., 4—8 October 1979. Terman, L. M., ed. Mental and Physical Traits of a Thousand Gifted Children, Genetic Studies of Genius, vol I. Stanford: Stanford University Press, 1925. Thorndike, E. L. "The Interests of Adults: II: The Interrelations of Adult Interestsfi Journal of Educational Psychology 26 (September—October 1935): 496-507. Tyler, Ralph W. Basic Principles of Curriculum and Instruction. Chicago: University of Chicago, 1949. U. S. Department of Health, Education, and Welfare. The Education of Adolescents: The Final Report and Recommendations of the National Panel on High School and Adolescent Education, Pubn. OE-7600004. Washington, D. C.: U. S. Government Printing Office, 1976. Van Manen, Max. "An Experiment in Educational Theorizing: The Utrecht SchoolJ' Interchange 10 (1978-1979): 48—660 Ware, William IL, and Garber, Malcolm. "The Home Environment as a Predictor of School Achievement." Theory Into Practice 11 (June 1972): 190—195. Watson, Kathryn J. The Going Places Classroom: A Community Involvement Program of Action Learning for Elementapy Students, Research Monograph No. 23. Gainsville, Florida: Florida University, P. K. Yonge Laboratory School, 1977. Webster's Seventh New Collegiate Dictionapy. Springfield, Mass.: G. and C. Merriam Co., 1970. Whitaker, Urban G. "Assessors and Their Qualifications." In Experiential Learning: Rationale, Characteristics, and Assessment, pp. 189-223. Edited by Morris T. Keeton. San Francisco: Jossey—Bass, 1977. 113 White, Robert W. "Motivation Reconsidered: The Concept of Competence} Psychological Review 66 (September 1959): 297—333. . "Sense of Interpersonal Competence: Two Case Studies and Some Reflections on Origins." In The Study of Lives. Edited by Robert W. White. New York: Atherton Press, 1966. Wigginton, Eliot. The Foxfire Book. Garden City, N. YJ Anchor Books, 1972. . Foxfire 2. Garden City, N. Y.: Anchor Books, 1973. . Foxfire 6. Garden City, N. Y.: Anchor Books, 1980. Willingham, Warren W. "Critical Issues and Basic Requirements of Assessment." In Experiential Learning; RationaleL_Characteristics, and Assessment, pp. 224- 244. Edited by Morris T. Keeton. San Francisco: Jossey—Bass, 1976.