.7 ‘cuqb—r %. ‘ .J od\-vo-at-u .‘Vr . lMgAuL-I V A n—v-wx '\ 2 ”2:. - . Ina-31:4 ,' ‘: , “n’ju ' 2 v13} If: ~> v :3 f“- " :3 A. ‘- ' ‘ .1 .- . .. o ' .'.‘U" J'Mu'31'~ ‘1' __ . " t" ’. a ., ~k. - . -" 14, . '. ‘9 ’_u ‘_ - Ln" ‘ "7 -‘-'. " . . :2": ., ~ Eur -, . , - . . ~. .. _ t. ‘ -' .3 ' -, ’~:-. . , - h " ~ '- -: - . " ‘ . v u. - . - " .v . '- . . ‘f A, "' yvqrg :4- . - : .,. . J ._ ‘ . , , , ,‘ _ i. ‘ , 4rzl$ 33‘5" ._ I ‘ ‘ A v - '4', w 9 '-" w " - “ 4 a -' - . -.*’i"n'£=‘:.l~2' . . = [. v 'Lwfig‘fl' 33‘ -Hv&%:. "Fifi .;.-.~.';;' "“1" 5'64 v v )1 ‘ :2...- n '5‘1. 1”?" ‘ ‘x4 . O I .g 1:. , . . 'u ' ‘ . ". , . .- ~ . A", _ t ‘ 3; ' .,'.' ‘ ' r ' ' A ' . I . . . * .- p . ' . s 'v.. ‘k‘ L. A. “2 . ' .,c i ‘ {._ I o A ‘I y l . ‘ I A v , . :4 p .. .. -‘ ~ «‘ 1' ?‘ .' ' 0 ' I \ ‘i ' ‘ Iv ' ' y. . ,g "‘ .. ‘ .,.' .4 " J r '1‘— . .l v‘. ~ . ‘.. n .th'gw MI” "'.P‘ V 1-“! ‘ ,;.. ”a ‘ .fi‘E', . . l ' .. ' . . w 2‘ ' ,, _....:.:.~: -. . . -. . . .'I 'l . .‘dtbp ,h ,. ._\ .nnIm. .. ,I _ an -. mm. ' ‘ .~ new 1 y \! wfimwh'u 11?." H, . . .513" ' ‘. 1 39333., “mtg." . .' _~"' ’0‘. .337: >.. -. "'3’" 3. v‘.: m I 1. 12.3%: . 52.» 7 » 4% ‘ ' IL! .» ’3", r. lard; 1&3” ' 'H‘rq J” J2“ L‘w '. = -fi1'u§3;1?’z.'.'=r§«-&' ”‘1?! 1“ ' '5 v i r) if. J'iéz‘: XI .‘ 4»N‘;"“ M 3-1"qu D “’1- ‘ V dd 1 -.~ I ' 7 gr 4. ’L é?” o . . ' ‘2’. P“ I \thi‘Efs. 56% I}. , d: 1W“ "‘.. ‘g u “C & v; ,‘ 15:. ‘ , ,'~ Am V“ x . z “ ‘1‘ ”a”: ‘ {Ii-@912" 4,, ' 5.232;; I 5‘ t .. ’_J . 'T‘fi'H” h". 4,]. .- - . .l - .fi, 5‘ 9‘, . ’ “J," '. . r ,- 3 '1 'L‘" “N High; $1141.: 31 4:4 .Ivsvé. 12' my; ' g... I hi ,0! in r'. ‘t! “45.2”" d.kfl§3aixhfl§ffi ‘1 A. ”.1, ‘ Ifi-Ii’ r. If‘ , iii‘ “Wt 5 {iv J" M; ._...t,.4,.§y;fi.q, , 45;; ! llllllflllIllgflllllfllllllHllHlJH/lIIJJHIIHIJHll 7180344 This is to certify that the thesis entitled Factors Associated with the Correspondence of Cancer Patient and Caregiver Reported Symptoms presented by Rachelle Kathryn Williams has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for M. S . degree in Epidemiology /%.D&Wflm/ Major professor Dflzfiz /4’7X 0-7639 MS U is an Affirmative Action/Equal Opportunity Institution LIBRARY l Michigan State University PLACE IN RETURN Box to remove this checkout from your record. TO AVOID FINE return on or before date due. DATE DUE MTE DUE DATE DUE 1M clam-9651).“ FACTORS ASSOCIATED WITH THE CORRESPONDENCE OF CANCER PATIENT AND CAREGIVER REPORTED SYMPTOMS By Rachelle Kathryn Williams A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF SCIENCE Department of Epidemiology 1998 ABSTRACT FACTORS ASSOCIATED WITH THE CORRESPONDENCE OF CANCER PATIENT AND CAREGIVER REPORTED SYMPTOMS By Rachelle K. Williams As more and more elderly cancer patients receive ambulatory care and remain at home, caregiving becomes an important issue presently and in the future. In order to investigate an aspect the the patient/caregiver relationship, concordance between the cancer patient and their family or friend caregiver on the presence or absence of fifteen common symptoms will be assessed. This concordance will then be used to determine if various caregiver or dyadic characteristics significantly predict the odds of disagreement by using multiple logistic regression techniques. The characteristics to be assessed include caregiver age, gender, living arrangements and marital status to the patient. In the univariate logistic regression, five symptoms showed at least one variable to be a significant predictor of disagreement. For poor appetite, nausea, trouble sleeping, dry mouth and coordination problems, caregivers 65 years and older were less likely to disagree with the patient. Additionally, for trouble sleeping, dry mouth and coordination problems, caregiver-patient dyads not married to and living apart from each other were found to more likely to disagree than those dyads married to and living with each other. Similar results were seen with the multiple logistic regression and will be discussed further in the paper as well as the implications for future research. Cepyright by RACHELLE KATHRYN WILLIAMS 1998 ACKNOWLEDGMENTS I would like to acknowledge my committee members, Dr. Barbara Given, Dr. Charles Given, and Dr. Dorothy Pathak, for their continuous help and support throughout the process. I would also like to acknowledge the students and staff who have assisted me in various ways with my thesis. Finally, this thesis would not have been possible without the recruiters and interviewers to gather the data. I would like to sincerely thank them. iv TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES ......................................................................................................... vi LIST OF FIGURES ........................................................................................................ viii BACKGROUND INFORMATION AND THEORETICAL FRAMEWORK .............. 1 METHODS .................................................................................................................... 9 Family Home Care for Cancer Study / Sample ......................................................... 9 Research Questions / Data Analysis ......................................................................... 10 Concordance ........................................................................................................ 10 Association between disagreement and caregiver characteristics ...................... 11 RESULTS ...................................................................................................................... 14 Research Question [A - Concordance ...................................................................... 15 Research Question 1 B - Sensitivity and Specificity Analysis .................................... 18 Research Question 2 - Multiple Logistic Regression ................................................ 22 Univariate Analysis .............................................................................................. 22 Multivariate Analysis ........................................................................................... 24 DISCUSSION ................................................................................................................ 27 APPENDICES ............................................................................................................... 36 ENDNOTES .................................................................................................................. 77 BIBLIOGRAPHY .......................................................................................................... 8O LIST OF TABLES Table 1 - Variables used for logistic regression ................................................. 36 Table 2 - Frequency of selected caregiver/dyad characteristics among sample ................................................................................................. 37 Table 3 - Summary statistics of frequency of symptom presence and concordance for entire sample ............................................................ 38 Table 4 - Overall patient/caregiver concordance ............................................... 39 Table 5 - Entire sample: patient/caregiver concordance .................................... 40 Table 6 - Male caregivers: patient/caregiver concordance ................................ 41 Table 7 - Female caregivers: patient/caregiver concordance ............................. 42 Table 8 - Caregivers less than 50 years: patient/caregiver concordance ............ 43 Table 9 - Caregivers 50-64 years old: patient/caregiver concordance ............... 44 Table 10 - Caregivers 65 years and older: patient/caregiver concordance .......... 45 Table 11 - Caregivers not married to and live apart from patients: patient/caregiver concordance ............................................................ 46 Table 12 - Caregivers not married to and live with patients: patient/caregiver concordance ............................................................ 47 Table 13 - Caregivers married to and live with patients: patient/ caregiver concordance ........................................................................ 48 Table 14 - Caregivers live with patients: patient/caregiver concordance ............ 49 Table 15 - Caregivers live apart from patients: patient/caregiver concordance ........................................................................................ 50 Table 16 - Caregivers married to patients: patient/caregiver concordance .......... 51 Table 17 - Caregivers not married to patients: patient/caregiver concordance ........................................................................................ 52 Table 18 - Sensitivity and specificity of the caregiver to the patient ranked by sensitivity ........................................................................... 53 Table 19 - Sensitivity and specificity of the caregiver to the patient by caregiver gender, ranked by sensitivity ......................................... 54 Table 20 - Sensitivity and specificity of the caregiver to the patient by caregiver age, ranked by sensitivity .................................................... 55 Table 21 - Sensitivity and specificity of the caregiver to the patient by relationship/living arrangements, ranked by sensitivity ..................... 57 vi Table 22 - Table 23 - Table 24 - Table 25 - Table 26 - Table 27 - Table 28 - Table 29 - Table 30 — Table 31 - Table 32 - Table 33 - Table 34 - Table 35 - Table 36 - Table 37 - Table 38 - Table 39 - Table 40 - Table 41 - Table 42 - Sensitivity and specificity of the caregiver to the patient by caregiver residence, ranked by sensitivity .......................................... 59 Sensitivity and specificity of the caregiver to the patient by caregiver relationship, ranked by sensitivity ....................................... 6O Univariate logistic regression to determine odds of disagreement for pain, fatigue and trouble sleeping ................................................. 61 Univariate logistic regression to determine odds of disagreement for GI symptoms ................................................................................. 62 Univariate logistic regression to determine odds of disagreement for respiratory symptoms .................................................................... 64 Univariate logistic regression to determine odds of disagreement for miscellaneous symptoms ............................................................... 65 Odds of disagreement for pain ............................................................ 66 Odds of disagreement for fatigue ........................................................ 67 Odds of disagreement for trouble sleeping ......................................... 68 Odds of disagreement for poor appetite .............................................. 69 Odds of disagreement for diarrhea ...................................................... 7O Odds of disagreement for constipation ............................................... 71 Odds of disagreement for nausea ........................................................ 72 Odds of disagreement for vomiting .................................................... 73 Odds of disagreement for weight loss ................................................. 74 Odds of disagreement for dry mouth .................................................. 75 Odds of disagreement for cough ......................................................... 76 Odds of disagreement for difficulty breathing .................................... 77 Odds of disagreement for coordination problems ............................... 78 Odds of disagreement for frequent urination ...................................... 79 Odds of disagreement for fever .......................................................... 8O vii LIST OF FIGURES Figure l - Comparison of relationship status and current living arrangements among 356 dyads ........................................................... 81 viii BACKGROUND INFORMATION AND THEORETICAL FRAMEWORK Cancer has consistently been called a disease of aging as more than half of all cancers are diagnosed in people age 65 and older.1 Therefore, cancer in the elderly population is a very important topic to be researched. In these patients, a significant number of symptoms have been documented and it is assumed that patients with cancer will inevitably suffer from certain symptoms some time throughout their illness. For example, it has been reported that approximately 70% of patients with cancer experience significant pain some time during the course of their illness.2 Many other symptoms have also been reported, including, but not limited to, nausea, poor appetite, insomnia, fatigue, cough, constipation, and diarrhea. Because caregivers are responsible for and interact closely with the patient, they greatly influence the patients’ quality of life. Physicians and nurses become informed of patients’ symptom distress directly through the patient and caregiver. Therefore, good communication between the caregiver, patient, and the healthcare providers becomes very important in managing symptom distress. Discrepancies between patient and caregiver views on symptomology may reflect misunderstanding or miscommunication, thus leading to inadequate symptom control, dissatisfaction with the caregiver role, and inadequate well-being for both.3 Therefore, looking at concordance of cancer patient and caregiver reports of symptom distress becomes a very important issue. Unfortunately, to date, there has been relatively little literature published discussing the issue of agreement between cancer patient and patient caregiver about the presence of various symptoms. 2 As increasing numbers of elderly and ailing persons choose to remain at home, caregiving for these persons becomes a very important issue. Caregiving in this study is defined as assistance of any kind given to the patient. Caregiving tasks include the amount and type of care provided by the caregiver. The tasks are typically defined as activities of daily living (ADL’s) (i.e., eating, bathing, dressing, toileting, etc.) and instrumental activities of daily living (IADL’s) (i.e., transportation, laundry, shopping, etc.). Additional tasks include emotional support, symptom management, and service utilization. Caregivers may assist because of chronic conditions, such as cancer or Alzheimer’s disease, or simply because of increased age and diminishing ability to perform certain tasks. In the present study, increased age combined with the presence of cancer has introduced the need for caregivers. Much of the research that has been conducted to date in the area of caregiving has generally emphasized the importance of family and friends for patient care and the challenges involved.4’5’6’7’8 However, a large portion of this research has had a tendency to investigate caregivers as a homogenous group which may result in hiding differences between groups of caregivers (i.e., age of the caregiver, gender of the caregiver, relationship of the caregiver to the patient).9 More recent literature has focused on gender ' ' - - 41 1 and it’s impact on the caregivmg rolel’2’3’ ' 0' 1 as well as caregiver-patient relationship (i.e., spouse versus non-spouse)."3’5’l2 Several important findings have stemmed from the literature focusing on caregiver gender and it’s effect on the caregiving role. The first finding is that the majority of caregivers to elderly persons are women and are likely to provide the bulk of the care.13 ’14’15’16 A second finding is that the type of care provided to 3 the recipient is often gender-based.l7 The gender-role socialization hypothesis argues that women are more vulnerable to effects of stress because of earlier socialization factors such as sensitivity to relationships, nurturing versus instrumental behaviors, illness behaviors, and coping styles and suggests that there are traditionally differences seen between the genders in various household tasks.lo Consistent with this suggestion, Curtis and Femsler (1989) found that male caregivers tended to help with household maintenance and repair and driving, while female caregivers were more likely to help with cooking, shopping and laundry.3 Corroborating literature includes that by Horowitz (1985)”, Stone and Short (1990)”, Chang and White-Means (1991)”, and Miller and Cafasso (1992)”). The third finding is that female caregivers consistently experience more burden or stress than male caregiverslz’l3 Issues that haven’t been researched as thoroughly and will be included for analysis in this paper are caregiver living arrangements (i.e., coresidence) and caregiver age. Coresidence has been touched upon briefly in papers by Chappell (1991) and Tennstedt et.al. (1993).20’21 Chappell, in particular, investigated three separate dyadic combinations and their effect on caregiving in terms of IADL and emotional support. The groups examined included caregivers married to and living with the patient, caregivers not married to but living with the patient, and patients receiving care but living alone. The key finding in this research is that living with someone as opposed to marital status was more important for assistance with IADL. One apparent limitation of previous caregiving research is the failure to examine various characteristics of the caregiver such as caregiver age, marital status, proximity to 4 the care recipient, and employment status that would seem to influence care.l Caregiver age, although not appearing in the literature frequently, should be considered when investigating the caregiver role for many reasons. Since all patients in this study are 65 years and older, caregivers in the same age group may build more of a rapport with the patient as they may have more in common. Age may also be indicative of employment status. Caregivers 65 years and older are more likely to be retired than younger caregivers, and therefore, may have more time to spend with the patient. Fitting and colleagues found that younger caregivers (<68 years) were less satisfied and more resentful of the caregiving role than were older (268 years) caregivers.22 It was also noted than male caregivers were typically older than female caregivers.” These findings could reflect differences between older and younger caregivers, or between male and female caregivers. Kurtz et.al., examined caregiver age in relation to concordance of cancer patient and caregiver reports of symptoms. Their findings suggest that age of the caregiver may not play an important role when looking at concordance. Although patterns in caregiver age were observed when looking at individual symptoms, there was no consistent pattern seen across symptoms.8 The present research will investigate caregiver age in the same manner. As previously mentioned, the issue of concordance between patient and caregiver on symptom presence or absence has not been investigated to a large extent. One study in particular addressing this topic is by Kurtz et al. The researchers investigated whether patient caregiver reports of the cancer patients’ symptoms were in agreement with reports by patients. They went further and looked at whether other variables, including patient 5 depression, caregiver depression, caregiver optimism, and perceived impact of caring on caregiver health, would explain discrepancies in the patients’ and caregivers’ reports. They found the overall agreement for all symptoms between patient and caregiver to be 71% and independent of the number of symptoms reported by the patient.11 Further, they observed the rate of agreement to be highest for fatigue and lowest for insomnia. Female caregivers were found to have a higher percent agreement and level of association with their patients than male caregivers. Several articles have been published specifically on the issue of pain. This is likely due to the fact that pain occurs in approximately one-third of patients receiving therapy for cancer and more than two-thirds with advanced disease.“25 Grossman et al., performed a study looking at the communication about cancer pain intensity between patients and their caregivers.l The caregiver in this study refers to the patients’ health care provider. Pain was assessed using the Visual Analogue Scale (VAS) which was given to both the patient and the caregiver. In general, the results of this study indicated that caregiver perceptions of patient pain are ofien dissimilar from those of the patient. The researchers found these differences to be most noticeable in patients with significant pain. A slightly different conclusion was reached by Hodgkins and colleagues in their study comparing patients’ and their physicians’ assessment of pain.26 The VAS was used to estimate pain. A correlation analysis revealed that prior to an invasive procedure, patients could predict the pain they would experience more accurate than their physician. The physicians better estimated this pain after the procedure had taken place. The authors concluded that patients may be better at predicting level of pain than their physicians, 6 however, the physicians’ estimates “appear to be accurate enough to allow them to give useful information about the degree of discomfort that a patient will experience during an invasive procedure.” The purpose of this study is to investigate whether cancer patient reports of selected symptoms are in agreement with the cancer patient’s caregiver reports of the same symptoms and whether certain factors, including caregiver age, caregiver gender, caregiver relationship to patient, and caregiver residence are associated with this agreement. The symptoms that will be investigated are nausea, pain, poor appetite, weight loss, trouble sleeping, fatigue, fever, difficulty breathing, cough, dry mouth, constipation, diarrhea, frequent urination, coordination problems, and vomiting. The questions posed by the study will attempt to be answered using existing data previously collected through The Family Home Care for Cancer -- A Community Based Model”. The relevant data consists of patient and caregiver reports of absence or presence of 15 symptoms collected at the first interview post-diagnosis. Patients and their caregivers were asked by trained personnel whether a particular symptom had been present or absent over the two weeks prior to the interview. In the present paper, it is hypothesized that given the traditional caregiving role women have taken in the past, there will be less disagreement between patients with female caregivers as opposed to patients with male caregivers regarding presence or absence of symptoms. This hypothesis stems from the fact that women are more likely to assist in the day-to-day activities as well as take on more responsibility for caregiving than males. Because of this, female caregivers will likely see or be more informed of 7 symptom distress of the patient more often than males. Further, it is hypothesized that increased agreement will be observed between patients and caregivers married to each other versus those not married to each other, between patients and caregivers coresiding versus those living apart from each other, and between patients with older caregivers as opposed to younger caregivers. These hypotheses have been derived from literature which has suggested that spouse caregivers ofien view their role as an accepted part of marriage and tend to be more committed than non-spouse caregivers. Further, spouse caregivers provide a greater range of assistance and more time spent on caregiving."3 The hypothesis that coresiding patients and caregivers are more likely to concord with each other on the presence or absence of symptoms was based on research by Chappell suggesting that living with the caregiver rather than being married to the caregiver was more important, in particular, for assistance with IADL.15 It is also presumed that more accurate and a greater amount of observance would occur with caregivers living with the patient as these caregivers will “see” more than those living apart from the patient. It was further hypothesized that older caregivers (as opposed to younger caregivers) would be more likely to agree with the patient on the presence or absence of symptoms. This was based on the findings of Fitting et al., of a more resentful attitude of younger caregivers than older caregivers. A contradiction was noted, however, in the fact that the present research hypothesized that older (versus younger) and female (versus male) caregivers would be more likely to agree with the patient. However, as noted by Fitting et al., men in the caregiving role are usually older than women. The present research and analysis will attempt to explain the associations. 8 The data to be used to investigate the hypothesized associations have previously been “cleaned”. The data has been gathered into a SPSS portable file to be converted to SAS format for analysis. METHODS Family Home Care for Cancer Study / Sample The Family Home Care for Cancer Study is longitudinal and utilizes patients aged 65 and older with a diagnosis of one of four cancers: breast, colo-rectal, lung or prostate cancer. The study is tracking the impact of the natural course of these diseases and their treatments upon the patient’s physical firnctioning, symptoms, medical care needs, use of services and the costs that are imposed upon the patient and their families. Telephone interviews are performed throughout the course of one year at pre-determined milestones to collect the information. Both patient and caregiver (if available) are interviewed at these milestones. In addition to the telephone interviews, self-administered booklets (SAB’s) are sent out immediately after the interview requesting return within two weeks. The SAB is sent to both the patient and caregiver and has firrther questions regarding patient and caregiver health and feelings. The data used in this report is based on telephone interviews of a convenience sample of 362 patients and their family or friend caregivers recruited by trained personal at hospitals or cancer centers throughout Michigan. All patients were 65 years or older. For dyads agreeing to participate, informed consent was obtained and interviews were conducted one month after surgery for patients having surgical treatment, or within two weeks of initiation of chemo or radiation therapy. Patients without caregivers were excluded from this report. 10 Research Questions / Data Analysis The following research questions will be addressed in this study: 1A. When looking at the presence or absence of symptoms asked of both caregiver and patient, what is the rate of agreement for the total sample, for male and female caregivers, for differing age groups of caregivers, for various caregiver-patient relationships, and for caregiver residence? 1B. What is the sensitivity and specificity of the caregiver’s report of symptoms as it relates to the cancer patient’s reported symptoms? 2. Are higher or lower rates of agreement for a given symptom associated with caregiver gender, caregiver age, caregiver relationship to patient, or caregiver residence? Concordance To determine the level of concordance between the patient and caregiver (research question 1A), several statistical methods were employed: percent agreement, kappa, sensitivity and specificity. For a given symptoms, percent agreement is defined as the number of dyads who agree on the presence or absence of a symptom over all dyads in the sample. The kappa statistic is designed to assess the level of agreement beyond that which may occur by chance. Kappa is determined by the following formula: Kappa = [(Observed)-(Expected)]/1-Expected. The observed agreement is the number calculated from percent agreement, above. The expected agreement is determined by the following formula: (No. cg state yes)(No. pt state yes) + (No. cg state no)(No. pt state no) (Total No. Dyads)2 ll Sensitivity refers to the proportion of caregivers who agree with the patient that a symptom is present, given that symptom is present, while specificity refers to the proportion of caregivers who agree with the patient that a symptom is absent, given that symptom is absent. These statistics were computed for each of the following groups: total sample, caregivers less than 50 years of age, caregivers 50 to 64 years of age, caregivers 65 years of age and older, dyads who live together, dyads who live apart, dyads who are manied to each other, dyads who are not married to each other, female caregivers, and male caregivers. Subsequently, groups were created to describe combinations of living arrangements and relationship. Thus, the statistics were also computed for following groups: dyads not married to each other and living apart, dyads not married to each other and living together, and dyads married to each other and living together. Only one dyad was described as married to each other and living apart, and will be included with dyads not married and living apart. Levels of significance for kappa values were also computed. Additionally, sensitivity and specificity of the caregivers response to the patients response were determined in order to get a feeling of agreement when presence of symptoms was reported by the patient versus agreement when absence of symptoms was reported by the patient (research question 18). Association between disagreement and caregiver characteristics For analysis of research question 2, multiple logistic regression will be utilized to determine whether the odds of disagreement differ for the various levels of a given caregiver characteristic with and without adjustment for other potentially confounding 12 variables. For each symptom, univariate logistic regression analysis was run for each of the caregiver characteristics: age group of the caregiver (CAGE), relationship of the caregiver to the patient (RLTNCAT), gender of the caregiver (CSEX), and residence of the caregiver (CCURLIV). The variable groups are listed in Table l. Cross-tabulations were created for these two variable, and it was discovered that only one dyad fit into the category of spouses living apart. This cross-tabulation is presented in Figure 1. In an effort to sort these variable out, the two variables, CCURLIV and RLTNCAT, were combined into three indicator variables, as briefly mentioned above. The first indicator variable included spouses who lived together, and was termed S_TOG. The second included non-spouses who lived together, and was termed NS_TOG. The third variable included non-spouses who lived apart and was termed NS_APART. A group was not created for spouses who lived apart as there was only one dyad fitting into this category. It was included in the category NS_APART. In the univariate logistic regression, analyses were run using the original variables (CCURLIV and RLTNCAT) first, then again, using NS_TOG, NS_APART, and S_TOG as predictors of disagreement (reference = S_TOG). A series of models was then run using multiple logistic regression to assess the effect of one variable while controlling for one or more other variables. These models used the indicator variables for relationship and living arrangements as opposed to the separate variables, CCURLIV and RLTNCAT. Further, the caregiver age variable was used as an indicator variable, instead of a continuous variable, as caregivers were separated into three age categories. 13 In the multivariate logistic regression, the following models were run: disagreement = caregiver age + relationship/living arrangements disagreement = caregiver age + caregiver gender disagreement = caregiver gender + relationship/living arrangements disagreement = caregiver age + caregiver gender + relationship/living arrangements RESULTS Descriptive characteristics of the sample are summarized in Table 2. The total sample size is 362 dyads, although, since there is missing information for some dyads, this number may be lower for various variables. Of 351 caregivers with available data on age, 12.8% (n=45) were less than 50 years of age, 23.4% (n=82) were 50-64 years of age, . and 63.8% (n=224) were 65 years and older. Information was available for the entire sample on gender of the caregiver. 23.5% (n=85) were male and 76.5% (n=277) were female. Of 356 caregivers with available data on living arrangements, 82.9% (n=265) live with the patient and 17.1% (n=61) live apart from the patient. Of this same number, 74.4% (n=265) are married to the patient and 25.6% (n=9l) are not married to the patient. The last two variable were combined to describe the overall caregiver-patient relationship. Of the 356 caregivers with available information on both marital status and living arrangements, 74.16% (n=264) are married to and live with the patient, 0.28% (n=l) are married to and live apart from the patient, 8.71% (n=31) are not married to and live with the patient, and 16.85% (n=60) are not married to and live apart from the patient. The frequency of the fifteen symptoms as reported by the patient and caregiver is given in Table 3. Fatigue was the most commonly reported symptom by both the patient and the caregiver (62.4%, 67%, respectively), followed by pain (46.7%, 51%, respectively). Fever and vomiting were the least frequently reported symptoms by both the patient and the caregiver (<7 %). l4 1 5 Research Question [A - Concordance Rates of agreement between the cancer patient and patient caregiver were computed two ways: percent agreement and kappa. Each method is briefly described in the Methods section. Statistics were computed for the entire sample, and for the various sub-groups of interest. Further, concordance was determined for two variations of the original data set. First, concordance was computed using only those dyads for which all patients and all caregivers responded to all the questions. Second, the analyses were run using all dyads in the data set regardless of whether both the patient and the caregiver or just one responded to a given question. The results from the two analyses are given in Tables 5-17. The results obtained from both analyses did not differ, and therefore, all dyads will be used in subsequent analyses. The overall agreement for the entire sample and for sub-groups is presented in Table 4. Statistics are calculated first for those dyads who responded to all fifteen symptoms, and again for all dyads. When just dyads responding to all symptoms is analyzed, the overall agreement is 81.24%. When all dyads are used, this agreement changes slightly to 80.78%. For the remaining sub-groups, the change in agreement is minimal. It is apparent that caregivers living apart from the patient have a lower agreement that those living with the patient (76.43%, 81.71%, respectively). Similarly, non-spousal caregivers have a lower agreement than spousal caregivers (77.75%, 82.03%, respectively) and agreement increases as caregiver age increases from <50 years to 50-64 years to >64 years (74.37%, 80.23%, 82.27%, respectively). No difference in agreement is seen, however, between male and female caregivers (80.49%, 80.56%, respectively). 16 For the entire sample (see Table 5), the highest rate of agreement was seen with fever (95.28%) and vomiting (95.29%), followed by poor appetite (86.74%), diarrhea (86.46%), coordination problems (86.43%), nausea (85.08%), constipation (83.70%), difficulty breathing (83.15%) and cough (80.94%). The remaining symptoms had rates of agreement below 80%, with the lowest percent agreement associated with dry mouth (64.72%). Kappa statistics ranged from a low 0.23 (dry mouth) to a high .64 (poor F appetite). All statistics were significant at 0t=0.05. The complete results are shown in Table 5. For all sub-groups analyzed, the highest percent agreement was consistently seen with fever (>91%) and vomiting (>91%). In the group of patients with non-spousal caregivers (see Table 17), high agreement was also seen with difficulty breathing (>91%). The lowest percent agreement was consistently seen with dry mouth (<69%). However, for the group of patients with spousal caregivers (see Table 16) and for the group with caregivers aged 65 and older (see Table 10), percent agreement was lowest for fatigue (68.40% and 67.26%, respectively). Caregivers consistently agreed more frequently on fever and vomiting and less fi'equently for dry mouth and fatigue regardless of age, however middle-aged caregivers (see Table 9) had higher agreement for fatigue (78.05%) than did older (67.26%, see Table 10) or younger caregivers (66.67%, see Table 8). In general, increasing agreement is seen as caregiver age increases. Similarly, caregivers agreed more frequently on fever and vomiting regardless of gender, although male caregivers (see Table 6) had slightly higher agreement for 17 vomiting than did female caregivers (97.62% vs. 94.58%) (see Table 7), while female caregivers had slightly higher agreement for fever than did male caregivers (95.64% vs. 91.12%). Both male and female caregivers disagreed the most frequently for dry mouth (<67%). Female caregivers tended to agree more frequently for difficulty breathing than did male caregivers (84.12% vs. 80%), while male caregivers tended to agree more frequently for frequent urination than did female caregivers (77.65% vs. 68.56%). Some differences were seen between caregivers not married to and living apart from the patient (see Table 11), caregivers not manied to and living with the patient (see Table 12), and caregivers married to and living with the patient (see Table 13). Each group appeared to agree most frequently for fever and vomiting (>90%) and least fiequently for dry mouth (<70%), as seen in previous groups. Caregivers not manied to and living apart from the patient also had a low rate of disagreement for trouble sleeping (56.67%). Caregivers not married to and living with the patient had a lower agreement than did the other two groups for poor appetite and cough. This same group had a higher rate of agreement for difficulty breathing than the other two groups. For trouble sleeping, dry mouth, constipation, frequent urination and coordination problems, there was increasing agreement as caregivers went from not married and living apart from the patient to not married and living with the patient to married and living with the patient. Although CCURLIV and RLTNCAT were not used as separate indicators, agreement was still calculated for reference. The overall percent agreement for dyads living together was 81.71% (see Table 14). For dyads living apart, the percent agreement dropped to 76.43% (see Table 15). For dyads living together, the highest rate of l8 agreement was again seen with fever (95.24%) and vomiting (95.58%), while the lowest was seen with dry mouth (68.47%) and fatigue (69.39%). For caregiver-patient dyads living apart from each other the highest agreement was again seen with fever (96.67%) and vomiting (95.08%), however, the lowest was seen with dry mouth (47.46%) and trouble sleeping (56.67%). Spousal caregivers showed their highest agreement with fever (95.56%) and r' vomiting (96.28%) and the lowest with dry mouth (68.89%) and fatigue (68.4%) (see Table 16). For non-spousal caregivers, fever again had the highest agreement (94.44%), it along with difficulty breathing (92.61%). The lowest agreement was again seen with dry mouth (52.22%), however, trouble sleeping was next lowest (60.44%) (see Table 17). Research Question 1 B - Sensitivity and Specificity Analysis Sensitivity and specificity was computed for the entire sample, and for various caregiver subgroups (<50 years, 50-64 years, >64 years, male, female, spouse, non- I spouse, live with patient, and live apart from patient). Recall, sensitivity refers to the proportion of caregivers who agree with the patient that a symptom is present, given that symptom is present, while sensitivity refers to the proportion of caregivers who agree with the patient that a symptom is absent, given that symptom is absent. For these analyses, the patient was considered the “gold standar ”. The results are shown in Tables 18-23. For the entire sample (Table 18), the highest sensitivity was seen with fatigue (79.20%) and diarrhea (79.73%). The lowest was seen with dry mouth (40.82%) and coordination problems (41.03%). The highest specificity was seen with fever (97.92%) 19 and vomiting (97.05%), while the lowest was seen with fatigue (53.33%). The remaining specificities were greater than 71%. In the case of gender (Table 19), male caregivers had the highest sensitivity for diarrhea (85.71%) and the lowest for difficulty breathing (30.00%). Female caregivers, on the other hand, had the highest sensitivity for fatigue (79.76%) and the lowest for coordination problems (40.00%). With the exception of pain, fatigue, and trouble sleeping, specificities were all greater than 80% for both males and females. For the exceptions, specificity varied from 53.2% to 74.66%. Male caregivers had higher sensitivity for nausea and vomiting than female caregivers, while female caregivers had higher sensitivity for poor appetite, cough, weight loss, frequent urination, fever, and difficulty breathing than male caregivers. In general, female caregivers tend to have higher sensitivity than male caregivers for more symptoms. Table 20 outlines the results seen for the various caregiver age categories. Younger caregivers had higher sensitivity for constipation (83.33%) than did middle-aged (55%) or older caregivers (59.57%), although the specificity was similar. Middle-aged caregivers had a higher sensitivity for fatigue (89.1%) than did younger (78.79%) or older caregivers (70.21%), however, the specificity for all groups was relatively low. Middle- age caregivers also had a much higher sensitivity for vomiting (80%) than the other groups (~55%), although the specificity for all three groups was very high. For frequent urination, weight loss and dry mouth, older caregivers had higher sensitivity than the other groups. For coordination problems, middle-aged caregivers had a much lower 20 sensitivity, while for fever, younger caregivers had a much lower sensitivity. No overall trends were observed between caregiver age categories. The next group of caregivers analyzed consisted of three groups with results presented in Table 21: caregivers not married to and living apart from the patient (N S_APART), caregivers not married to and living with the patient (N S_TOG), and caregivers married to and living with the patient (S_TOG). For NS_APART, the highest sensitivity was seen with poor appetite (85%), and the lowest with dry mouth (26.67%). All specificities were greater than 83% with the exception of fatigue (42.11%), pain (69.23%) and trouble sleeping (70.27%). For NS_TOG, the highest sensitivity was seen with trouble sleeping (87.5%) and the lowest again with dry mouth (40%). Pain, fatigue and trouble sleeping again had lower specificities (56.25%, 62.5%, 60.87%, respectively) as well as cough (68.42%). The rest had a specificity greater than 75%. For S_TOG, the highest was seen with diarrhea (81.48%) and the lowest seen with coordination problems (38.1%). Again, as seen with the other two groups, pain, fatigue and trouble sleeping had lower specificity (74%, 55.66%, and 73.86%, respectively). The rest were all above 82%. For diarrhea, weight loss, trouble sleeping, frequent urination and dry mouth, NS_APART caregivers had much lower sensitivity than the other two groups. NS_TOG caregivers appeared to have much higher sensitivity for trouble sleeping, difficulty breathing, cough and coordination problems than the other two groups. S_TOG caregivers had a higher sensitivity for vomiting and a lower sensitivity for poor appetite as compared to the other two groups. Overall, caregivers not married to and living apart 21 from the patient tended to have lower sensitivity than caregivers married to and living with the patient and caregivers not married to and living with the patient. As previously mentioned, although CCURLIV and RLTNCAT were not used in the final analyses, sensitivity and specificity were still calculated for reference. Results pertaining to caregiver residence are presented in Table 22. Results for caregiver relationship are presented in Table 23. For those caregivers living with the patient, sensitivity was highest for diarrhea (81.97%) and lowest for coordination problems and dry mouth (44.25, 44.44, respectively). For caregivers living apart from the patient, highest sensitivity was seen for fatigue (85%), while the lowest was again seen for coordination problems and dry mouth (27.27%, 26.67%, respectively). Caregivers who lived with the patient had a much higher sensitivity for diarrhea, trouble sleeping, vomiting, weight loss, frequent urination, coordination problems, and dry mouth than for caregivers who lived apart from the patient. Conversely, caregivers who lived with the patient had a much lower sensitivity for poor appetite than for caregivers who lived apart from the patient. Spousal caregivers had highest sensitivity for diarrhea (81.82%) and the lowest for coordination problems (40.91%). For non-spousal caregivers, poor appetite showed the highest sensitivity (85.71%), while dry mouth had the lowest (31.82%). For vomiting, trouble sleeping, frequent urination, and dry mouth, spousal caregivers had much higher sensitivity than non—spousal caregivers. However, spousal caregivers had much lower sensitivity than non-spousal caregivers for poor appetite and difficulty breathing. 22 Research question 2 - Multiple Logistic Regression Univariate Analysis Multiple logistic regression was run first for each symptom analyzing only one variable at a time. These results have been compiled and organized as follows: pain, fatigue and trouble sleeping are displayed together on Table 24, as they are commonly grouped together for cancer patients; gastrointestinal symptoms are displayed together on Table 25; respiratory symptoms are displayed together on Table 26; the remaining symptoms are grouped together as “other” on Table 27. Of the fifteen symptoms analyzed, only nausea, poor appetite, trouble sleeping, dry mouth, and coordination problems had one or more significant predictor variables. For nausea and poor appetite, age was determined to be a significant predictor of disagreement (OR=0.614, p=0.010 and OR=0.638, p=0.028, respectively). As the age of the caregiver increases, the odds for disagreement decrease. In other words, there tends to be a higher rate of agreement between patients and older caregivers versus patients and younger caregivers, which may suggest a spousal phenomena. This same trend is also seen for trouble sleeping, dry mouth, and coordination problems. Additionally, for these three symptoms, caregiver residence and caregiver relationship to the patient also become significant predictors of disagreement. Caregivers who live with the patient are at less than half as likely to disagree with the patient on the presence of these symptoms versus caregivers who live apart from the patient (OR=2.098, p=0.010; OR=2.560, p=0.001; OR=2.729, p=0.004, respectively). Further, caregivers and patients who have a spousal relationship are also approximately half as likely to disagree with each other on the presence of the above 23 symptoms as opposed to those with a non-spousal relationship (OR=1.815, p=0.018; OR=2.120, p=0.002; OR=2.222, p=0.012, respectively). When indicator variables for caregiver/patient relationship and living arrangements were used in the model, similar results were seen. Symptoms having significant associations between disagreement and the predicator variables included trouble sleeping, dry mouth, and coordination problems. In this model, the reference characteristics were patient and caregiver having a spousal relationship and living together. The two comparison groups were patients and caregivers who were not married, but lived together, and patients and caregivers who were not married and lived apart. As seen for trouble sleeping, when comparing the first group (not married and live together) to the reference, only one change is made: non-spouse patients and caregivers are compared to spouse patients and caregivers. Here, no significant difference is seen (OR=1.294, p=0.528). The next comparison is with the living arrangements (living together compared to living apart), since the previous comparison was not significant. For this comparison, there is a significant difference (OR=2.159, p=0.009) and the only change was in the living arrangements. In other words, the significance is coming from the living arrangements, and not the relationship of the caregiver to the patient. For the remaining symptoms (other than nausea, poor appetite, trouble sleeping, dry mouth, and coordination problems), none of the variables tested achieved significance. 24 Multivariate Analysis In an effort to further understand the associations, multiple logistic regression was performed using multivariate models. The results from this analysis are presented in Tables 28-42. As in the univariate analysis, the only symptoms with significant predictors of disagreement were nausea (Table 34), poor appetite (Table 31), trouble sleeping (Table 30), dry mouth (Table 37), and coordination problems (Table 40). Non- significant results for the remaining symptoms are displayed in the remaining tables. Once again, the age group to which the caregiver belongs is consistently seen as a significant predictor of disagreement in each of the five symptoms mentioned above. For nausea and trouble sleeping, there are no other variables than caregiver age group that significantly predict disagreement, as in the univariate analysis. For nausea, caregivers aged 65 and older were less likely to disagree with the patient than caregivers less than 50 years of age when controlling for caregiver gender (OR=0.435, p=0.045). Although the odds ratios for disagreement in the other models are also decreased (in other words, less likely to disagree/more likely to agree), they were not significant. Poor appetite has a similar pattern as seen with nausea. Caregivers aged 65 and older were about one third less likely to disagree with the patient than caregivers aged less than fifty (OR=0.358, p=0.025). This association was seen only when controlling for caregiver gender. Once again, although the odds ratios for disagreement in the other models were also decreased, they were not significant. 25 Similar to what was observed with nausea and poor appetite, for dry mouth, caregiver age only achieves significance when the gender of the caregiver is controlled for. In other words, after adjusting for any extraneous effects caregiver gender may have on disagreement, caregiver age remained significant. Here, caregivers 65 years and older are about two fifths less likely to disagree with the patient than caregivers less than 50 years (OR=0.403, p=0.007). In the other three models, however, there is a consistent association seen with caregivers who are not married to and live apart from the patient. When controlling for caregiver age, this group of caregivers was about two times more likely to disagree than caregivers married to and living with the patient (OR=2.053, p=0.059). When controlling for caregiver gender, this group of caregivers was almost three times more likely to disagree than the reference group (OR=2.726, p=0.001). In the full model, controlling for caregiver age and gender, this group of caregivers was again about two times more likely to disagree than the reference group (OR=2.066, p=0.057). For trouble sleeping, caregivers aged 65 and older again are less likely to disagree with the patient only when controlling for caregiver gender (OR=0.428, p=0.013). Similarly, caregivers not married to and living apart from the patient are more likely to disagree with the patient when compared to caregivers married to and living with the patient only when controlling for caregiver gender (OR=2.192, p=0.009). For coordination problems, increasing age of the caregiver is a significant predictor of disagreement in all models. When controlling for relationship/living arrangements, caregivers aged 65 and older were shown to have approximately one third the chance of disagreeing as caregivers less than 50 years old (OR=.303, p=0.029). When 26 gender of the caregiver was controlled for, this association was even more pronounced, and showed up in caregivers aged 50-64 as well. Caregivers 65 and older were about one fourth as likely to disagree as caregivers 50 years or less (OR=.23 8, p<0.001) while caregivers aged 50-64 were about two fifths as likely to disagree as caregivers 50 years or less (OR=.415, p=0.047). In the model controlling for caregiver gender as well as relationship/living arrangements, caregivers 65 and older were about one fourth as likely to disagree as caregivers less than 50 years (OR=0.287, p=0.025). Also showing up as a significant predictor of disagreement is relationship/living arrangements. When controlling for caregiver gender, caregivers who were not married to the patient and who lived apart from the patient were almost three times more likely to disagree than caregivers who were married to and lived with the patient (OR=2.865, p=0.004). DISCUSSION Patients with cancer and their caregivers need to be better educated about how to manage symptom distress. This is particularly true for elderly cancer patients, as they tend to experience more symptoms. Several symptoms were investigated in this study. One of the reasons various symptoms such as fatigue, pain and dry mouth are present in such high frequencies is that the patients in this study had undergone some form of treatment for their cancer during the month prior to interview. Although data was not available on which patients had which type of treatment, the symptoms mentioned above have been known to be associated with chemotherapy. Fatigue, in particular, had the highest prevalence, which is consistent with previous literature, however, the rate of agreement was relatively low, not consistent with previous research. This could possibly be due to the fact that since it is often present concurrently with treatment, it may be appearing only in intermittent phases, whereas other symptoms may be present on a more continuous cycle. One unique aspect of this study is the utilization of both percent agreement and kappa coefficients to demonstrate concordance. Percent agreement would at most be considered a crude estimate of concordance as it does not take into consideration that agreement which would occur by chance. The kappa statistic is the most popular measure of agreement which provides estimates beyond chance.28 It is important to realize, however, that because kappa takes into account chance agreement, it is affected by the distribution of data across the categories, i.e., “prevalence” of yes’s and no’s as indicators 27 28 of symptom absence or presence. To illustrate, consider the results seen for the symptom, coordination problems, for the entire sample. The percent agreement was calculated as 86.43%, which is relatively high. The kappa statistic, however, is calculated at 0.32, which would be considered relatively poor. Compare this kappa to that seen with poor appetite for which a similar percent agreement is seen (86.74%). The kappa statistic here is much higher, at .64. This occurs because the distribution of yes’s and no’s is different. This difference is seen primarily in the distribution of yes’s (i.e., prevalence). For poor appetite, 25.1% of the patients (17.7% of the dyads) responded “yes”, while for coordination problems, only10.8% of the patients (4.4% of the dyads) responded “yes”. This difference in distribution directly affects the amount of chance agreement, thus affecting the value of kappa. With higher prevalence, the results less likely to be due to chance. In this sense, by calculating both percent agreement and kappa, one is able to evaluate agreement as it applies to distributions with high and low prevalence of “yes’s. Although not all variables emerged as significant predictors of disagreement in the regression analysis, caregiver age and relationship/living arrangements did. Five symptoms had at least one of these variables as significant predictors. Caregiver age was consistently found to be a predictor of disagreement with few exceptions, which were not significant. Recall what was observed: Caregivers aged 65 and older were less than half as likely to disagree on the presence or absence of symptoms when compared to caregivers less than 50 years of age. Caregivers aged 50-64 years, were also found to be less likely to disagree, although significance was observed only with the symptom, coordination problems. Several possibilities exist for explanation. First, persons 65 years 29 and older are more likely to be retired and able to spend more time with the patient. When looking at the distribution of caregiver age, only 12.8% are less than 50 years, 23.4% are 50-64 years, and 63.8% are 65 and older. More time spent with the patient would reasonably imply more accurate observance of symptoms. Further, consider the fact that all the patients included in this research are at least 65 years of age and the majority of dyads are married to each other (74.4%). It therefore seems reasonable to assume that as the majority of caregivers are married to patients 65 years and older, they are more likely to be 65 years and older as well. As previously discussed in the framework section, spouses are more likely to be committed to the caregiving role than are non-spouses. If the spouses in this study were more likely to be 65 years and older, this could potentially explain the finding that younger caregivers were more likely to disagree. In fact, 91.5% of all spousal caregivers are 65 years and older. Issues of marital status and living arrangements of the dyad are important in caregiving research. However, these two characteristics were highly correlated with each other. By including both in the model, each variable adjusted for the effect of the other variable, neither was a significant predictor of disagreement. Therefore, combinations of the two, previously described as NS_APART, NS_TOG, and S_TOG, were used to represent the dyads in this study. Significance was seen with trouble sleeping, dry mouth and coordination problems. When CCURLIV and RLTNCAT were analyzed as separate variables (not controlling for other variables) the odds ratios are highly significant, when the variables were merged into combinations describing the dyads, the odds ratios were even higher and more significant. Here, the reference group was dyads married to and 30 living with each other. The two comparison groups were dyads not manied to and living with each other and dyads not manied to and living apart from each other. The latter group significantly predicted disagreement. It might be asked how does one determine where the significance is coming from, since not married/living apart is being compared to married/living together. In the model, when dyads not manied/living together are compared to dyads married/living together (thus, only comparing marital status, since living arrangements are not changed), there is no significant difference. However, when the marital status remains, and the living arrangements are changed (living together changes to living apart), a significant difference is seen: the odds ratio drastically increases from 1 to greater than 2 (p<0.01). The conclusion to be drawn, therefore, is that living arrangements are more important in predicting presence or absence of symptoms than marital status is, although the two cannot be separated. This interpretation must be taken cautiously as the fourth group of dyads, patients married to each other but living apart from each other, was not available to analyze. The conclusions reached are logical as caregivers who live with the patient, whether as a spouse, friend, or other relative, would observe more and be informed more of what is happening physically with the patient. Simply being a spouse, regardless of whether or not that spouse lived with the patient, would not seemingly predict disagreement. As was seen, being a spouse, when analyzed individually came in as a significant predictor of disagreement, because the majority of spouses live with the patient. In dyads whose caregivers live apart from the patient, much of the interaction may occur via telephone and the majority of the face-to- face interaction likely occurs during short periods of time during which the caregiver is 31 probably not able to observe all of the symptom distress. Further, they are not able to observe any symptom distress occurring during the night. Although gender of the caregiver has been shown to be important in various caregiving roles, the present research did not find it to be a significant predictor of disagreement when analyzed individually. This is consistent with research done by Kurtz et al.5 Although predictors of disagreement can be explained, as above, for some symptoms, analyses with other symptoms showed no significant predictors of disagreement. Pain, fatigue and trouble sleeping have long been known as symptoms commonly associated with cancer and often are grouped together for research purposes.”'9’3O In the present analysis, significant predictors of disagreement were observed for trouble sleeping, but not for pain and fatigue. For trouble sleeping, caregivers 65 years and older were shown to be less likely to disagree when compared to caregivers 50 years or less, while caregivers not married to and living apart from the patient were shown to be more likely to disagree with the patient than caregivers married to and living with the patient on the presence or absence of symptoms. This may be explained by the fact that caregivers living with the patient would be more likely to observe and/or discuss with the patient sleeping habits. Further, as the patients are all 65+ years, spousal caregivers are more likely to be 65+ years as well. In this analysis, all but one spouse lives with the patient and, thus, the previous argument could be assumed. Pain and fatigue did not show any significant predictors of disagreement. Fatigue is quite often associated with chemotherapy and radiation treatment and may represent only an 32 intermittent phase of symptom distress as opposed to symptoms which are more ubiquitous3 1, thus possibly explaining a lack of association. Pain may be a symptom that is relative in terms of how it is defined. What the patient refers to as pain, the caregiver may not. Several gastrointestinal symptoms were analyzed in this study, including poor appetite, diarrhea, constipation, nausea, vomiting, and weight loss. Of these, only poor appetite and nausea had significant predictors of disagreement, and of the variables assessed, caregiver age was the only significant predictor. Recall that caregivers 65 years and older were less than half as likely to disagree as their younger counterparts (less than 50 years). Vomiting, weight loss, and diarrhea may often be associated with chemotherapy or radiation therapy and only be present for short, intermittent periods of time. Perhaps, in the case of diarrhea and constipation, the symptom distress is not discussed between the patient and the caregiver, or is not acknowledged, because of the personal nature of the symptom. Weight loss may occur insidiously and not be as noticeable a symptom as others may be. The respiratory symptoms analyzed included dry mouth, cough, and difficulty breathing. The symptoms difficulty breathing and coughing, in particular, have been shown in the literature to be common in lung cancer patients”. Many times, these symptoms are associated with other respiratory conditions commonly seen in lung cancer patients”, such as emphysema, and may be attributed by the caregiver to this particular condition. If these other conditions have been present for a long period of time it is likely that the symptoms also may have been present for a long period of time, and may have 33 become less noticeable with time, potentially explaining a lack of association. In particular, the nature of the questionnaire may be a problem, as it asks whether the symptom has been present in the past two weeks. This may imply a change from the past times to the past two weeks. In the case of dry mouth, caregivers 65 years and older were found to be less likely to disagree with the patient than the younger comparison group. Models run for cough and difficulty breathing, did not show any significant predictors of disagreement. A difficulty here is that likely, coughing and breathing trouble are much more prevalent in the lung cancer portion of the patients in this study. Unfortunately, at the time of this study, cancer site data was not available to run the analyses on the stratified data. In this case, perhaps predictors of disagreement would emerge. In the case of coordination problems, significant predictors of disagreement may be present due to the fact that this symptom would be quite noticeable. In fact, a significant predictor of disagreement was found with caregivers 65 years and older and with caregivers not married to and living apart from the patient. This is reasonable, as the coordination problems would be more noticeable to a caregiver living with the patient rather than apart from the patient. Frequent urination is a symptom that is very common in the geriatric population in general and may not be attributed to the cancer, therefore, masking any associations. Fever is as noticeable a symptom as coordination problems is and may not be readily recognized. This could potentially explain a the lack of association observed here. There are several limitations to this study which need to be discussed. Foremost is the argument that the same regression analysis is being run for those symptoms with 34 similar responses. Perhaps a more appropriate method of analysis in similar future research would involve analyzing groups of symptoms that are similar to each other. However, the present study is exploratory in nature, and a grouping of symptoms may mask the differences that are seen. A second limitation is that the analyses were run on the entire sample as well as stratified by caregiver characteristics, but not stratified by site of cancer. At the time of this study, data on the site of cancer was not available, however, future attempts to research this area should include this data. In this study, the sample sizes for each group after being stratified by cancer site may have been too small to see significant differences between subgroups. It is also important to note that this study investigated the research questions symptom by symptom, and not by number of symptoms. Future studies may benefit by including this further aspect in the research. A third limitation in this study is the small number of independent variables analyzed. Other variables that may introduce more findings include, but are not limited to, caregiver race, patient gender, stage of patient’s cancer at diagnosis or interview, caregiver and/or patient employment status, and what type of occupation the caregiver holds. As well, subjective variables such as caregiver and/or patient depression, optimism, comorbidity of the patient, and health status of the caregiver may provide further information in the area of caregiving and concordance. Based on the findings of this study, directions for future study have emerged. As mentioned previously, a limitation of the study is the exclusion of many important independent variables. The inclusion of these variables for analysis would likely provide a better understanding of the caregiver-patient relationship. Further explanation of the 35 findings of this study may be found in the number of symptoms reported. This was not analyzed in this study, however, there is a possibility that as the number of symptoms reported increases, disagreement may be affected. Finally, an aspect that should be considered in future research is the site of cancer. Different cancers are often associated with different symptoms, different severity of disease, and different types of caregiving needed. Perhaps different patterns of disagreement would emerge within each patient group based on site of cancer. This research is important to patients and their caregivers because it gives insight into the caregiver-patient relationship by looking at both individual caregiver characteristics as well as dyadic characteristics. The findings illustrate that certain groups of caregivers (aged 65 and older, living with and married to the patient) can reasonably act as proxies for the patient for information on presence or absence of symptoms. However, this study shows that misunderstanding does exist between the patient and the caregiver when certain characteristics are present (caregiver is less than 50 years old, caregiver lives apart from and is not married to the patient). The inability to understand and appropriately manage symptoms can add to patient and caregiver distress and burden. Education of both the patient and the caregiver on the various aspects of symptom distress is key to a better understanding of the caregiving role and to better communication. APPENDICES 36 :8st FEB wE>= ES 9 BEE .523ch - QOHIm Eozma 5? 9:2. new 2 8E9: 6: 82850 - OOHIm Z Eozma So: team 9.3.. new 2 ooEmE 8: $29ch - Hm= acme 830%-.8: 295m S «.93 Hm= 530 830% 29: S OmV *mUZmemmémDHRHm .2532 2.3500 H64 224 63.8 351 100 Caregiver Gender Male 85 23.5 Female 277 76.5 362 100 Caregiver Residence cg lives with pt 295 82.9 cg lives apart from pt 61 17.1 356 100 Caregiver Relation to Patient Spouse 265 74.4 Non-spouse 91 25.6 356 100 Residence/Relationship Combination NS_APART 61 17.1 NS_TOG 31 8.7 S_TOG 264 74.2 356 100 38 00.0 and N50 mm.m0 n0. _0 _0.m 00.0 00.0 00>00_ 00.0 and 00.0 0 9.0m 00.; 3.0 00.: 00.2 0.803000 000000000000 00.0 00.0 mm.0m :00 00.00 84% 00.2 0030 00000003 00000000 cod 00.0 3.00 m _ .mw ands 3&0 20— 05mm $05005 53055 00.0 mmd 00.0_ 00.00 3.0 000: om.0m 000.0. 03000 cod mmd wmwm $1.00 03:V n0.0_ owfim 00.00 5002 E 00.0 ~00 mwdm 0_.00. mmdm «.de cmdm omém 000: £303 cod 0.0 21.0 0m.m0 3.00 0:0 00.0 3.0 w000080> cod mmd .00.: 00.3 000.0. m 0.9 00.8 00.: 000002 cod 00.0 0000 05mm 00.00 00.3 000m ow. 0m 000000002000 00.0 N00 000.2 00.0w 0 0 .00. 0000 00.3 ovdm 0000.85 00.0 0.0 0N? 00.0w 00.00 0.: 00.8 o _ .mm 0000000. 0000 00.0 000 00.00 00.00 00.? 00:8 00.; 00.3 900005 030000. 00.0 mmd nvdm mm .00 0000 $00 00.0 9&0 033000 00.0 $0 3.0m 00.2. mmdm mm?” 00. 0 0 00.0.0 .0000 0203-0 0&3. 00086 000mm .00 A03 $0.0 000030000 000030-50 .x. =000>O 000mm 00 000mm .00 00000900.? 503 .00 0000000.? 50$ o0 03:000. 00:00 000 00000000000 00.0 00000000 000090000 .00 000000000 .00 000000000 Egm .m 030,—. 39 Table 4. Overall patient/caregiver concordance DS-l DS-2 Group Overall % Agreement Overall % Agreement (weighted) Total 81.24 80.78 Caregiver Residence: with patient 81.93 81.71 apart from patient 76.19 76.43 Relationship spouse 82.14 82.03 non-spouse 77.1 1 77.75 Caregiver Age < 50 years 73.71 74.37 50-64 years 80.63 80.23 > 65 years 82.42 82.27 Caregiver Gender Male 81.75 80.49 Female 80.76 80.56 DS-l - includes dyads with a response for every symptom DS-2 - includes all dyads 0:0»: =0 000205 - «-mQ 8000:0000 bog :8 8:802 0 5:5 000% 020205 - TmO Aowfigm vofimmoav 40 00.00 00.0 0805 00.00 00.0 _0.0 :00 00.00 00.0 00.0 000000> 00.00 00.0 00.0 _00 00.00 00.0 00.0 08200.0 000050.00: 00.00 00.0 00.0 000 00. :0 00.0 :0 00000:: 0000020 00.00 00.0 00.0 000 00.00 00.0 00.0 00055 00.00 00.0 00.0 000 0: .00 00.0 00.0 0000000000 00.00 00.0 00.0 000 0_ .00 00.0 00.0 5008 05 00.00 00.0 00.0 000 00.00 00.0 00.0 00000 0: .00 00.0 00.0 000 00.00 00.0 00.0 0020020 0:00:05 00.00 00.0 00.0 000 0.00 00.0 00.0 .300 00.00 00.0 00.0 :00 00.00 00.0 00.0 000000 00.00 00.0 00.0 000 00.00 00.0 00.0 03085 20020 0_ .00 00.0 00.0 000 00.00 00.0 :00 00.3 £003 00.00 00.0 00.0 000 00.00 00.0 00.0 300090 .000 00.00 00.0 00.0 :00 030 00.0 00.0 .000 00.00 00.0 00.0 000 E .00 00.0 00.0 000002 08w< 0\0 0200, 8000M : 8:90 0\0 33> 0&0M Boas—mm .: .0 040d @0008 70: 8:023:00 00333805000: 638:0 2::m .m 033. 00000 =0 000220 - 0-00 8009.500 E05 00.0 00:008. 0 53> 000000 000205 - TmQ 41 Aowfiofi 003E030 mm. 5 00. S =80>O 00.00 00.0 00.0 00 00.00 00.0 00.0 05080> 00.00 00.0 000.0 00 00.00 00.0 V0.0 0803000 :000500000 00.00 5.0 00.0 00 00.00 00.0 «0.0 800:0: 0:003:00 00.00 00.0 000.0 00 00.00 00.0 000.0 00:55 00.00 00.0 00.0 00 00.00 00.0 00.0 5:800:00 00.00 00.0 8.0 0000 00.00 0.0 00.0 0000:: in 00.00 00.0 ~00 00 5.000 00.0 00.0 00:00 00.00 0. _ .0 2 .0 00 00.00 00.0 8.0 @2085 53055 N: .5 00.0 $0 00 $.00 00.0 3.0 00800 3.00 00.0 _ m .0 vw 00.00 8.0 00.0 0030000 00.: 00.0 3.0 30 00.: 00.0 00.0 @3005 03:80. 00.00 00.0 3.0 vw 00.00 00.0 0000 0004 20003 00.00 00.0 00 .0 00 00.00 00.0 00 .0 0000030 0000 00.00 00.0 3.0 0.0 v0.00 00.0 0.00 :3— ;00 00.0 00.0 00 00.00 00.0 00.0 000002 000w< 0\0 020g 0090M : 000W< 0\0 0:03 0000M 80.08% -0 -0 wad: 000u5 700 0050000800 0023300503 ”00023000 0.02 .0 0300. 00000 :0 mo020.0 - 0-00 0:000:30 b0>0 00.0 00:800.. 0 505 00000 000205 - TmQ 00000020 00003030 42 00.00 00.00 :85 00.00 00.0 00.0 000 00.00 00.0 00.0 000000> 00.00 00.0 00.0 000 00.00 00.0 00.0 082080 000050080 00.00 00.0 00.0 000 00.00 00.0 00.0 000000: 0000020 00.00 00.0 00.0 000 00.00 00.0 00.0 000.005 00.00 00.0 00.0 000 00.00 00.0 00.0 0000000000 0: .00 00.0 00.0 000 00.00 00.0 00.0 500:. 00 _0.00 00.0 00.0 000 00.00 00.0 00.0 00000 0: .00 00.0 00.0 000 E .00 00.0 00.0 0020020 0.8005 00.00 00.0 00.0 000 00.00 00.0 00.0 .260 :000 00.0 00.0 000 3.00 00.0 00.0 000000 00.00 00.0 00.0 000 3.00 00.0 00.0 050020 0.0080 00.00 00.0 00.0 000 00.00 00.0 00.0 003 00003 00.00 00.0 00.0 000 00.00 00.0 00.0 3000000 .000 00.00 00.0 00.0 000 00.00 00.0 00.0 000 00.00 00.0 00.0 000 :00 00.0 00.0 000002 000w< X. 0290 0000M : 000w< 0\0 029 0000M Eofiém -0 -0 0.0-0 0000-5 .00 0050000800 0300000005000 0000030000 0.080.,“ <- 0300. 43 A0w000>0 0050005 00000 =0 000205 - 0-00 50005000 b0>0 000 00:00.00: 0 :53 00000 000205 - TmD 00.00 _0.00 000000 :00 00.0 00.0 00 0: .00 00.0 00.0 000000> 00.00 :0 00.0 00 00.00 00.0 00 .0 0.020000 0000500000 _0.00 00.0 00.0 00 00.00 00.0 00.0 0000000 0000020 00.00 00.0 00.0 00 00.00 00.0 00.0 80:00 00.00 00.0 00.0 00 00.00 00.0 00.0 0000000000 0000 00.0 _0.0 00 00.00 00.0 00.0 .0008 000 00.00 00.0 00.0 00 00.00 00.0 00.0 00000 00.00 00.0 00.0 00 00.00 _0.0 00.0 0020020 008000 00.00 00.0 00.0 00 00.00 00.0 00.0 00000 00.00 00.0 0: .0 00 00.00 :0 E .0 000000 00.00 _0.0 00.0 00 00.00 00.0 00.0- 0.00020 0.00000 00.00 _0.0 00.0 00 00.00 00.0 00.0 003 00003 00.00 00.0 00.0 00 0000 00.0 00.0 00008.00 0000 00.00 _0.0 00.0 00 00.00 00.0 00.0 0000 00.00 00.0 00.0 00 00.00 _0.0 00.0 000002 000w< 0\0 020> 0000M : 000m< 0\0 020> 0000M 5000§m -0 -0 .000|0- 000u5 0-00 00:00:00:00 0053000050000 00000 on :05 000— 000>mm0000 .w 030,—- 44 38:30 080325 080 8 80220 - 0-8 839:8 bo>o Sm 80288 0 505 80080 80205 - TmQ 8.8 8.8 0805 800 000 :0 8 300 000 :0 80008> m _ .8 $0 80 8 8.8 80 000 202080 0002058 8. K 00.0 30 8 3.8 000 80 80200 050020 2 .8 000 80 8 8.8 00.0 80 8:85 8.8 00.0 80 8 8.: 00.0 :0 0008028 00.8 80 80 8 8.8 80 80 5008 an 800 00.0 80 8 8.8 00.0 80 0808 E. 8 000 80 8 80. _ 8 00.0 80 82880 0.886 8.8 00.0 80 8 8. a 000 80 0260 80.80 00.0 $0 8 8.8 00.0 80 2803 8.8 000 80 8 8.8 00.0 $0 80088 2008.0 8.8 00.0 80 8 80. _ 8 00.0 80 $3 2803 8.8 000 80 8 8.8 00.0 80 20002 .80 8.8 00.0 000 8 80.8 000 80 .80 8.8 000 80 8 80.8 000 80 03.82 08w< 0\0 02? 030M : 02w< 0\0 33> chasm angmm -a A. g Ens 0.89 85000808 5330855300. ”0.0 who» 00.3 803980 .o Bash 45 0080 =0 $0220 - 0-8: 599:8 bo>o Sm 8:288 0 553 000% 80505 - TmD 390850 085805 swam wig =0.5>O 00.3 cod and MNN no.8 ood 3d w55:8> mndw ood wmd vmm mvdo ood N06 85208: 5:050:80 00.? cod med 3N $.06 cod 0.0.0 55005.3 50:00:05 8.5 006 0.0.0 vum wosw cod «0.0 0050005 ~38 cod mmd VNN :38 ood mmd 5:058:00 88 00.0 80 08 8.8 000 80 5008 0: 00mm cod 00.0 van mndw ood de nmsoo mwéw cod 3.0 vmm 00.0w cod 00.0 m5£095 b.3055 $.00 cod $0 03 8.3 ood 3.0 :33 03b cod de mam 00.00 cod and “Swag 3&0 00.0 3.0 mmm mug? cod and w5aoo0m 03:0; hmdw cod who mmN oodm cod $6 804 Ewfi? 2 do 006 $0 0mm 3% 00.0 00.0 300%? :00: m0. 2. 00¢ 23 man 05:. cod 93 50m 002% 00.0 $6 «mm 00.3 00.0 $6 08:02 02w< 0\0 050> 0&0M : 02w< .x. 050> 0&0M Eogmfifi .: -a .N.|m| 88$ 70: 8:00.588 0053830530: ”80.: 0:0 0:00» we 0:053:00 .3 030,—. 46 A0w000>0 005893 0080 =0 80220 - 0-0: 59:58 be; :8 8:83: 0 £05 00080 000505 - 7mm $00 8.8 00.05 00.8 00.0 80 G 8.8 00.0 80 $008> 00.8 80 20 00 8.: E0 000 052080 80050.80 :8 000 80 8 8.8 000 80 0000000 080020 8.8 000 8.0 s 8.8 000 80 8055 8.8 00.0 S0 6 8.8 000 30 8000088 00.8 :0 000- 8 00.8 80 000 .088 b: 00.: 000 80 G 8.8 00.0 80 0008 00.: 00.0 80 S 8.8 80 8.0 0000020 202.05 800 000 80 00 800 000 80 :38 8.8 «00 80 G 8.8 000 80 08000 8.8 80 000 00 2.8 000 S0 05088 2008.0 3.8 00.0 80 s 8.8 00.0 80 83 3003 8.8 000 E0 6 8.8 000 80 200000. :80 8.8 00.0 $0 3 8.8 000 $0 .000 8.8 00.0 000 G :8 000 80 03002 02w< .x. 020> 0&0M : 02w< 0\0 020> 0550M Eowmgm -a .: mfll 3010 E: 8500888 0388000500: ”3:000: 89m 80:0 9»: 0:0 8 05:08 8: 0:953:00 .2 030.0 47 380 0.0 $0220 - 0-8 83088 Ego 8.0 8088: 0 505 0.0080 8022: .. 7mm Aow000>0 08.365 8.8 00.8 0805 8.00 00.0 8.0 8 8.8 8.0 8.0 05053, 0_ .8 00.0 8.0 8 8.8 00.0 000 £2020 000050.80 00.08 8.0 8.0 8 8.08 8.0 00.0 0000000 080020 8.00 00.0 8.0 8 8.8 8.0 00.0 80.55 8.8 00.0 8.0 8 8. 8 8.0 80 000000200 8. 8 2 .0 8.0 a 8.8 8.0 8.0 58:. an 8.08 00.0 8.0 8. 8.8 8.0 00.0 00:00 8.8 00.0 00.0 8 8.8 00.0 00.0 002085 $885 8.8 00.0 8.0 8 8.8 00.0 8.0 550 8.8 00.0 8.0 8 8.8 00.0 8.0 000000 8.8 00.0 8.0 8 8.00 00.0 8.0 05088 20:08 00.00 00.0 8.0 00 8.8 .00 8.0 $3 2003 8.8 00.0 00.0 8 8. 5 00.0 8.0 20003 500 8.08 8.0 8.0 8 8.8 8.0 00.0 000 8.8 00.0 8.0 8 00.; 00.0 8.0 8302 008.4. 0\0 2.0? 0.33. : on< 0\0 030> 0&0M 83%—mm A. -q filmn. fins 78 00000000000 03880005009 ”3:003 505 o>= 000 8 03.208 Ho: 838200 .2 030,—. 48 $3 a 83.05 - $9 899:? bog 8m 85%“: m at? $53 wows—2: - 7mm Emacs 35395 0 fl .mw 5.3 :895 omda cod 36 men omdo cod mod wcmzao> v93 cod ado vow omdw cod _m.o mac—noun 2235280 3.: cod 36 gm 3.? cod $3 c2338 Eugen v53 cod 8.0 gm 36w cod Nod 8:55 3.3 cod mmd V3 91% cod 36 5:33:00 de0 cod omd vow 3.2. cod Ed 5:08 ED :Q 85 $6 EN 3% 85 $6 $80 mmdx cod 93 EN omdw cod mvd magmas £355 3.? cod who vow 8.3 cod cod 85m 3.8 cod mmd mom ovwc cod mmd oawzwm wmdn cod mvd mom ends cod mvd weave—m 0520; beds cod de mom omdn cod who 33 2&6? mafia cod wed vow oo.$ cod wed 8:093 Sam vmén cod wed vow oven cod 36 5mm cméw cod mmd 3m owdw cod Rd «3st 08w< °\° 33> «gum : 02w< .x. 02? «aux ESQ—Him -q A. mm Swans 7mm 8568280 uozwobwobaouaq ”35:3 53> o>= Ba 9 BEE 8033qu .2 2an 49 898 a. 8835 - 3a 803E? bo>o 8m 0338.. a £5 39:8 8335 - 7mm Aowfiog @3385 :8 8.; =35 8.8 8.8 8.8 8N 8.8 8.0 8.8 masco> 8.8 8.8 28 8m 2 .8 8.8 88 £288 885280 M: .8 8.8 8.8 38 8.8 8.8 8.0 88%: 882m 2 .8 8.8 8.8 mg 8.8 8.8 8.8 8355 8.8 8.8 8.8 m8 2 .8 8.8 8.8 8:88.80 $8 88 8.8 8w 8.8 8.8 one 5.58 to 2.8 8.8 m3 8w 8.8 8.8 8.0 826 8.8 8.8 :8 m8 2 .8 8.8 8.8 8885 8885 8.8 8.8 one am :8 8.0 8.8 .28 8.8 8.8 8.8 gm :8 8.0 2.8 0888 8.8 8.8 8.0 an 8.8 8.8 8.8 880% 038; 2.8 8.8 moo am 88.88 8.8 :8 :3 £33 8.8 8.8 8.0 m8 8.8 8.8 8.8 3:83 .88 8.8 8.8 88 8m 8.: 8.8 8.8 :3 8.8 8.8 So am 8.8 8.8 8.0 53.82 oohw< o\o 33> 33M : 02w< o\o 33> «gum 889:3 -a A. Nwd. Sans 73 0282033 Simohwobcoamq ”3:33 53> o>= £02380 .3 2an 50 23% EN $8.2; - an ESE—Sm ago .8 3:88.. a 53> at“? $335 - TmQ Aowfiozw 3:365 SE 2.2 .355 8.3 cod 3.0 S 3.8 86 3o mange, 8.2 one 2 .o 8 Q. E who 8.0 3285 832688 38 So So % 2.8 cod go 535.5 283$ $3 cod $0 5 $9 cod o3 «£55 8.8 8d :8 S at cod 3% 858880 3: :6 :5. % 8.3 go :5 58:. to 8.: 8d one 6 3.; cod woo fisou 8.: 8d as S 2? So one @385 $885 $3 2:. 3o 8 8.3 cod 3o 53m 3.? 8d 3o 6 00% :3 one 2.ch Sow woo 3o 8 2 .3 3o :3 mcaooa 295; 3.? 00¢ 93 3 3K cod as 33 Emma N3” 23 who 6 3.5 cod N3 2:33 88 R? 8d 23 S S? cod 2% can 8. a 8d £3 6 :3 8.0 3.0 832 02m< o\o 03:2, 893‘ a 08w< .x. 33> ammam anfizm -a -q mlwd aqua 78 8:350:00 qumoBobcouaq ”35:8 Gob tuna o>= mquwoSU .2 2an 51 m8? EN 3305 - Ea 839.8? bog 5m 88%“: a 5m? $3.8 wows—05 - 7mm 33.5% Bfiwfigv mo.mw E .3 =Eo>o wmdo cod mod aom 36o cod wed magmas, 3.3 cod 36 ohm 36w cod wad mac—noun :oumfiEooU 3.9 cod 33 com wads cod 33 gamut: $25on 3.3 cod Ed EN no.3 cod mod 3:55 Eéw cod m We cum 36w cod mmd 8:35:00 236 cod omd ohm owdc cod Rd 5:08 b9 cod» cod 36 ohm maaw cod nmd nwsoo 3.3 cod mvd cum 3 .mm cod mvd magmas .932.th omwo cod wmd chm coda cod cod 55m 3.3 cod mmd com mode cod cmd oswcam mmdn cod 9% com wads cod mic wEQQBm 03:8... 3.2. cod mmd oom 3.3. cod mmd 304 Emma? 2mg cod Nod cum 8.5 cod $6 0:695 80m 2.: cod wvd com 3.: cod 93 53 Oman cod de ohm no.3 cod de $ng 023.. X. 2:? 39$ c oohw< °\o 83> mamaM Eoumaxm .a -Q g Gmmnfi 7mm 859880 Bzwofiobaoua ”$5ch 8 3%.: 8033.80 .2 £an 8:0 00 $0205 - 0-8 829900 Ego 8.0 00:09.2 0 53, 000% 020205 - TmD 52 Aowega 32365 8.8 0 _ .R 0826 00.00 00.0 80 N0 0000 00.0 00.0 00285 S .8 00.0 8.0 a 00.: 00.0 8.0 2209.0 0085208 t .00 _0.0 00.0 00 :8 00.0 8.0 0085.5 050080 030 00.0 $0 00 8.; 00.0 00.0 8055 8.8 00.0 :0 N0 00.8 00.0 $0 00800800 8.00 :0 v0.0 00 8.00 8.0 00 .0 5:08 be 00.8 00.0 $0 8 00.8 00.0 00.0 00000 3.00 00.0 00.0 00 00.8 00.0 00.0 00208.0 5.855 3.8 00.0 00.0 00 00.8 00.0 8.0 0300 8.8 00.0 :0 N0 2.8 00.0 8.0 808“. $00 0. .0 2 .0 a 8.00 8.0 m. .0 050020 280.0 00.00 00.0 00.0 a 8.00 00.0 00.0 $3 2053 030 00.0 000 N0 8.00 00.0 $0 30003 .000 8.00 00.0 3.0 00 00.8 00.0 00.0 500 mm. 8 00.0 8.0 00 8.8 00.0 8.0 8802 08w< 0\0 on?» «gum a 08m< 0\0 33> 0990M Eogqim -0 -0 mm GT8 78 8:098:00 8>Rob8>cowmq ”0.53330 8:30-82 .2 033. 53 mm. 5 dev 5:08 DO 8. a mo. 2‘ 080395 5:05.880 modw 3 .Vm c2855 :83on 3. a wwém wfifimofi bagga— No.3 ~03 030m 6.3 made 084 EwfiB 33 3.8 5&ch mm . K v0.00 wcaooi 03:0; Vmww 3.3 83090800 8.3 w _ .8 @5055 3.3 mhwo 08:02 3.3 «.02. 830%? 80m cm . K 3.00 can mm.mm ends ozmzmm 3 .3 mhdb 00::me bfimfiam bfimzmcom EoEEAm £23050 3 03:8 60:3 05 3 BZmoSo 05 m0 @650on 98 bm>£mcom .3 030,—. 54 $39.80 2083 a: 0.80 202 00.00 00.00 082020 000050.80 00.00 00.00 055020 032005 R. :0 00.00 6008 E 00.00 00.00 0050 00. :0 00.00 000005 050020 00.8 00.00 0000005 08002.0 0 _ .00 00.00 002320 £02.05 00.00 00.00 082020 00086800 00.00 020 003 20003 :8 00.00 5:08 5 0:0 00.00 000002 00.00 00.: 003 5063 00.00 :00 0250 00.00 00.00 00000 E. E 00.00 030020 208: 00.00 8.00 300000. 800 00.00 R00 00000 .000 00.00 00000028 00.00 00.00 000_eo> .08 3.00 050020 2000; 00.00 00.00 000000008 00.00 00.00 0508; 00.: 00.00 500 00.00 00.: 000000 0:0 :00 300000. 0000 0:0 00.; 500 00.00 0000 8055 00. 00 00. 3 8032 8.00 00.00 80000 00.00 :00 3055 006$“.on bSEmcom Eowmfixm bmommoomm ESEmcom 89953 533050 3 @838 .uovaow 833.80 3 E038. 05 3 833.80 05 mo $05890 can bmfizmaom .3 030,—. 55 $00 00.2 0.82080 000200.08 00.00 00.00 5005 E 00.: 0_ .00 £005 0.5 00.00_ 00.00 .300 00.00 00.00 8802 00.00 00.00 00000.8 0000020 00. _0 00. 0.0 800005 080020 00.8 $.00 0.03 £003 0_ .00 t .00 002820 $885 00.00 00.00 202080 0080000000 00.00 00.00 0250 00.00 00.00 0508.0 0.0080 00.00 00.00 00800800 8.00 020 008000, 00.8 00.00 0508.0 20000.0 00 .00 :00 00000 8.00 00.00 080 2003 00.8 :._0 002085 008005 8.00 00.00 0008 00.00 00.00 80002 00.00 00.: 20003 .000 00. 0 0 8.8 200000. .000 00.00 00.8 00089» 8.8 00.00 0:080 00.00 :00 80.55 00.00 00.8 500 00.00 00.3 500 _0.00 8.00 80:05 00.00 2.00 000000 _0.00 8.00 00800800 gofioomm bEzmcom EoEENm €2.06on bEfi—Bm Scan—mm Elmo» 3.3 8% 348% omv owflozwwzmlo $3338 3 tau—:8 awn $29.80 3 «:38 2: 3 $3908 05 mo bmoumoam 93 33383 .8 033. S6 8.3 8.: «83 Z 8.? nmsv mEofioa 5335800 523 3.3 waEEo> “3.8 3% magmas £385 amdo mmém 85m oodw Nmém wcfiofim 03:0; 8.; Ram couaumcoo mfion 3.8 5:08 E m fl .5 31% nwzou $.05 mmdo Eun— voda node 8:ng nméw £33 304 Emmokx cows N046 gamut: Heaven omdo 5:2. unmann— Su. 3 mugmw 0583‘ 80m £050on bréibm 899:3 83> ch Hgmoao @288 cm 2an 57 8. 8 8.8 588 E 8.8 8.8 588 be 8.8 8.8 8855 85828 :8 8.8 @288 885280 8.2: 8.8 .38 8.8 8.8 88%: 888; 2.8 8.8 8:85 8.8 8.8 8888 225: 8.8 8.8 88888 :8 8.2. $3 833 8.8 8.8 33 5303 8.8 8.8 m=28o> 8.8 8.8 @288 885280 8.8_ 8.8 $5.,” :8 8.8 882 8.8 8.8 8885 832.85 8.8 8.8 880 2.8 8.8 880 8.8 8.8 088.8 8. a 8.8 8:55 8.8_ 8.8 8835 £885 :8 8.8 8.0.82 5 _ .8 :8 8:55 8.8 8.8 88888 8.8 :8 388%.. 88 8.8 I .R 88 8.8 8.8 58 _ as. 8.8 088.8 8.8 8.8 8888 288... 8.8 8.8 3:83 88 bmommoomw bSEmcom 8395mm $050on bSEmaom anmém 550on 0263582 62 tlmo< 3583me 82 833233 3 coy—:8 .mEoEowg wE>EQEE3§2 3 25:8 05 8 53380 05 mo b85028 Ea brfiifim AN 2an 58 coda o_.wm £53on cozmanzooo Qumm oaév 5:08 DO 8.8 8.2 @385 £885 wodw omdm corset: Souven— amfio code 85m mm. 5 mmdo swaou omsw made 3.3 E303 3.3 $60 3333. 80m Nodw wmwo 5383800 mud» om.$ «.3st £65 3.3. wEQvBm 228C. 8.: 3.: 53 8.3 3.2. wGEEo> codm 8.: unmann— o fl .3 3. M w 8:55 3&6on brémmcom EoHaEmm baa. ozfiocsz 6:83 a 23 59 8.8 8.8 588 .5 8.8 8.8 £85 be :8 8.8 8288 8:88.80 8.8 8.8 8288 8:8280 8.8 8.8 8:88 882: 8.8 8.8 8:82: $885 8.2 8.8 8888 2:88 8.88 8.8 :22 :8 8.8 83 £83 8.8 8.8 8:88 8:88 8.8 8.8 888> 8.8 8.8 83 883 8.2: 8.8 88: 8.8 8.8 880 8.8 8.8 8885 8.885 2.8 8.8 888880 9 .8 8.8 880 8.8 8.8 288.4~ .58 8. 8 8.8 8:85 8.8 88 8.0.82 :8 8.8 882 E .8 8.8 8::8> 8.8 8.8 8:88.80 8.8 8.8 8885 2:88 8.88 :8 .88 8.8 8.8 88 _ :8 8. 8 08:8 3 .8 8.8 08:8 8.8 8.8 8:83 88 8 .8 8. 8 8:85 .3050on 555:3 Soap—Sm 5650on bFEmcom EoEEmm a o>= Eo:§5>§ :ofiowo: o>= “cosmmtozwgu $3558. 3 3:5: .8528: 8382.8 an 80an 05 8 829.80 05 mo bmofiooam :5 bm>Emcom .mm 033. 60 888-com 8 $2880 8:08 a 858,. 8:. U E. E 8. 8 m 588 an m _ .8 8.8 8288 885280 8.8 8.8 8:88 8888 8.8 8.8 £88 to 8.8 M: . S 8288 8:88:80 8. 8 8.8 8885 $885 8.8 8.8 .88 8.8 8.8 8:88 8888 2 .8 8.8 8:828 2:88 88.88 8.8 88 8.8 8.8 33 £83 8.8 :8 880 8.8 8.8 888$ 8.8 8.8 8:83 88 S .8 8.8 880 8.8 8.8 83 £83 8.8 8.8 8:8880 8.8 8.8 8:8:80 2 .8 8.8 8885 8885 8.8 8.8 8882 8.8 8.8 8882 8.8 8.8 8888 2:8; 88.88 8.8 8885 8.8 8.8 :8 3.8 8.8 58 8.8 8.8 8::8> m _ .8 8.8 88:8 8.8 8.8 88:8 8.8 :8 2:83 88 8.8 8.8 8:85 bmoumoam 33:8:me EBay—Em @650on bEESm Soap—mm 53288 3 “08—5: dimcouflo: 3 82:3 05 8 .3883 2: mo bmommooam :5 8.328% .mN 052. 61 mod n d E Emu—mama bfioumzfim 98 85:0 320m 82 was u mo 33 u a dddd amfia de wand Sad dgd- dvod mad; mmdd tam o>_=\om:o%-:oz 83 83 83 we §§§Rw w 5d mun; ommd Svd wand mmmd- Ewd cod; wood 3.30%-52 ddd.~ ddd .~ ddd.~ 3.:ch de wadfi :Kd omwd wmdd wood- 33d nod; Sod tags 35 8% .N 8% .N 8% .N we 95 mmwd Sad Sod- owdd wood wood. domd wmmg med 2&8th ddd .~ ddd .~ 25 .~ «B: m 3d bmvd nmwd- dead god m 5d- mmnd cwwd _N_d- 38» voA mhdd Smd Sod- 32d mend mmmd- :dd owed wand- ma?» vodm ddd .~ ddd .~ ddd .~ .983 dmv eggd MO a 039:— MO a cigé do a wEmoo—m unmask fig Amvo_nm5> :85qu0 232% uEDSS muzmgmxuk Eoumgm 0558830 “31330 mauve—m 03:95 was 2&th .53 8% EoEonamE mo ago 058.5on 9 commmflwou 23w“: Saw—gmaD .vm £an 62 mod n 5 E isomefi bfiosmzfim 08 85:0 vow—om 88 2:5 u mo 33 u a $8 :2 £2 £3 :3 mad- and 82 ~86. Ema geosavco: $2 :2 come 32 22 53 S S 2 3 33 .we oéasameo: Q8 .N § .N Q8 .N .3 §§§§ RS $2 88 £3 83 a; 22 $2 8.8 08838 83 e8 .N 83 2.8% $3 82 $3 SS 22 Sod- 22 53 $3- 5% 3: as .N e8 .N 23 .N .3 95 :3 3g 2.8- was 83 $3 on? as: $3 038$ 83 Se .N 83 23: :28 mm: $2 33 $2 $3 Sod can... 53- mag $A 2:5 EN 52 83V 82 23V $3 a: 33- e8» 3-3 e8 .N Q8 .N § .N 28a 3v 833 mo a 2.33 mo n 2%; mo a cosmmumcoo 3:95 852:3. Amvo_nmta> 533800 goon NEES; wutmxwxmz EoEEmm oumtoaogno Ho>mw2wu mEoEEAm 5 8m EoEoonamE mo muuo ganged 9 ~868th Bumme— BmflgiD .mm 03mg. 63 mod n d E “cocci—ma goofing 9a 853 woo—om one 53 n mo 93 u a mod omm._ w_md wand oo: wtd Emd mom; mmmd Ewan 2,5850%-8: omwd mo _ ._ mo _ d mm o .o wow.m mood momd omo._ Ewd .wS o>Eow=omméoa 25 o go o e8 .N we §S§§ wnwd omm._ oomd mm _ .o omog mood oomd nmw; ommd 820%-:0: ooo .~ ooo .~ ooo .~ 348% wmmd mmm._ momd wood wmog wmod omwd ommg mwmd tam 3: ooo .~ ooo .~ ooo .~ .MS 95 Sod mood hood- wwwd omod wwwd- womd _ _o.o mowd- vacuum ooo .N ooo .~ ooo .~ 36: oo_d mood owod- mmmd onwd omnd- omod oowd mood- mac» woA ww_d nmo d mmod- _mmd owmd wwod- omod wood m fimd- wan» wo-om go o as .N 8% o 28a RV o:_m>-m MO o Swain MO o 029;“ MO a $04 w:£Eo> «8st Goo—onwag :omtdoEoU Emma? floats; 8§$®- Saddam commuoaognu $3330 9:53 mm 03¢ mod N 5 3 80058me b35505 0.8 00580 o0Eom 0:8 025 u MO 92 u a ommd mow; wand ommd Rm; omwd Hood moo.m Edd .55 350053-52 mm _ .o mwmd moo. 7 wood mg .m mwnd momd nmw; ommd .moo 35353-52 0% o 83 08 .8 .02 352.3% mgd moo; Rod mood mmog wwwd mood omfim End 050%..52 ooo .~ ooo< ooo.~ 0.38am mo_.o moo; wuwd mmmd mom; ommd Sod oom.m owod 28—0 02. 0% .8 83 83 .09 05 ommd wmm._ Smd mond ommd mm _ d- m_ od 5 3d hood- 2080”— ooo .~ ooo< ooo.~ 0?: nwmd omod wowd- ommd mood wmwd- hood oowd Sod- 0.30» woA mood wwnd wwmd- mnwd mmod omod- omod mood wood. 50% wo-om ooo .~ ooo .~ ooo .~ 980% onv 035-9 m0 n 020>Aw MO a 0295 #5 M. 985005 nwsoo £52 @0308; 5085800 @3055 E 030.25. 00=0L0\0- 88983 08058? b.3858. 5o 80800505 .5 55 08580.0 9 82000308 oommwB 055383 .om 030H 65 mOd H .6 an #:62qume ~A=floflmflfim 0.3 mow—“:0 flow—Om 2:2 $8 n mo 93 u a Sad coo; mwoo wood 30.. oomo vooo QEN omog tau o>m=\om:o%-:oz ooo .N ooo .~ ooo .~ .MS §§m§cm® mvmo Kn; :mo 3 _ .o v2..— mnmo m _ o.o NNN.~ 03o omsoaméoz 8% .N e8 .N 25 .N aafim nmoo ooo.o mmoo- vwoo So; momo vooo aha woo." can 33 83 25 .N 33 we 9,: mono 3 2 B o .o goo ooo.o novd- ooo.o Boo ooo.o- 038$ ooo .~ ooo .~ ooo .~ m3: Sumo ofimo smoo- oomo vooo oomo- ooo.o 32. mm 2- mac» voA ooho mom; mono oovo omno ammo- ooo.o two goo- £8» woom 8Q .N § .N e3 .N 28% av unused MO a o=_m>-m MD a 029$ MO a 55m :23:ng £53on 3053.5» cemwamfioo 80:35 8355280 mBSES 8:3»me 889:3 mosmtouofianu 83980 $939me 3028:083— Hom EoEoEwmmmu we move 055.8% 9 commmemou owmmwB cantata: SN 2an 66 Table 28. Odds of disagreement for pain Caregiver Characteristics Statistics Reference Variable Comparison Variable 'E [3 OR p-value <5 0 years 1. 000 50-64 years 0798 0.450 0.103 >64 years -0.152 0.859 0.755 Spouse/live together 1. 000 Non-spouse/live together 0.213 1.237 0.641 Non-spouse/live apart -0.005 0.995 0.990 <5 0 years 1.000 50-64 years 0789 0.454 0.070 >64 years -0.161 0.851 0.658 Female 1. 000 Male 0.147 1.158 0.612 Female 1.000 Male 0.21 1 1.234 0.463 Spouse/live together 1. 000 Non-spouse/live together 0.206 1.229 0.627 Non-spouse/live apart 0.073 1.076 0.825 <5 0 years I. 000 50-64 years 0794 0.452 0.105 >64 years 0161 0.851 0.741 Female 1.000 Male 0.067 1.069 0.823 Spouse/live together 1. 000 Non-spouse/live together 0.222 1.249 0.628 Non-spouse/live apart 0.004 1.004 0.993 67 Table 29. Odds of disagreement for fatigue Caregiver Characteristics Statistics Reference Variable Comparison Variable L [3 OR p-value FT <5 0 years 1. 000 50-64 years -0.759 0.468 0.109 >64 years -0.279 0.756 0.557 Spouse/live together 1.000 Non-spouse/live together -0.472 0.624 0.328 Non-spouse/live apart -0.212 0.809 0.623 <5 0 years 1. 000 50-64 years -0.573 0.564 0.166 >64 years 0.028 1.028 0.938 Female 1. 000 Male -0.158 0.853 0.578 Female 1.000 Male -0.081 0.923 0.773 Spouse/live together 1.000 Non-spouse/live together -0.485 0.616 0.284 Non-spouse/live apart -0.l26 0.881 0.689 <5 0 years I. 000 50-64 years -0.769 0.464 0.106 >64 years -0.248 0.780 0.604 Female [.000 Male -0.194 0.824 0.504 Spouse/live together 1.000 Non-spouse/live together -0.497 0.609 0.304 Non-spouse/live apart -0.235 0.791 0.587 Table 30. Odds of disagreement for trouble sleeping 68 Caregiver Characteristics Statistics Reference Variable Comparison Variable LL [3 OR p-value < 5 0 years l 1. 000 50-64 years -0.383 0.682 0.374 >64 years -0.384 0.681 0.390 Spouse/live together 1. 000 Non-spouse/live together 0.181 1.199 0.679 Non-spouse/live apart 0.552 1.736 0.158 < 5 0 years 1. 000 50-64 years -0.692 0.500 0.071 >64 years -0.849 0.428 0.013 Female 1.000 Male 0.082 1.085 0.776 Female [.000 Male 0.067 1.070 0.814 Spouse/live together I. 000 Non-spouse/live together 0.271 1.311 0.511 Non-spouse/live apart 0.785 2.192 0.009 < 5 0 years I. 000 50-64 years -0.380 0.684 0.378 >64 years 0398 0.671 0.376 Female 1.000 Male 0.085 1.089 0.770 Spouse/live together 1. 000 Non-spouse/live together 0.195 1.212 0.663 Non-spouse/live apart 0.561 1.753 0.152 69 Table 31. Odds of disagreement for poor appetite Caregiver Characteristics Statistics Reference Variable Comparison Variable l [3 OR p-value <5 0 years 1. 000 50-64 years 0243 0.784 0.668 >64 years 0950 0.387 0.110 Spouse/live together 1.000 Non-spouse/live together 0.514 1 .672 0.325 Non-spouse/live apart -0.561 0.570 0.322 <5 0 years 1. 000 50-64 years -0.209 0.811 0.660 >64 years -1.026 0.358 0.025 Female 1. 000 Male 0.630 1.877 0.104 Female 1. 000 Male 0.51 1 1.667 0.159 Spouse/live together 1.000 Non-spouse/live together 0.861 2.366 0.072 Non-spouse/live apart 0.066 1.068 0.885 <50 years 1. 000 50-64 years -0.202 0.817 0.722 >64 years -1.088 0.337 0.075 Female 1. 000 Male 0.670 1.955 0.087 Spouse/live together 1. 000 Non-spouse/live together 0.590 1.803 0.272 Non-spouse/live apart -0.499 0.607 0.382 70 Table 32. Odds of disagreement for diarrhea Caregiver Characteristics Reference Variable Statistics Comparison Variable [3 OR p-value <5 0 years I. 000 50-64 years 0.445 1.561 0.471 >64 years 0.203 1.224 0.751 Spouse/live together 1.000 Non-spouse/live together 0.572 1 .771 0.274 Non-spouse/live apart -0.031 0.969 0.954 <5 0 years I. 000 50-64 years 0.409 1.506 0.467 >64 years 0.12] 1.129 0.818 Female 1. 000 Male 0.047 1.048 0.905 Female 1. 000 Male 0.114 1.120 0.759 Spouse/live together 1.000 Non-spouse/live together 0.474 1.606 0.339 Non—spouse/live apart 0.014 1.014 0.975 <5 0 years I. 000 50-64 years 0.452 1.571 0.464 >64 years 0.188 1.207 0.770 Female 1.000 Male 0.098 1.103 0.801 Spouse/live together I. 000 Non-spouse/live together 0.586 1.796 0.266 Non-spouse/live apart -0.021 0.979 0.969 71 Table 33. Odds of disagreement for constipation Caregiver Characteristics Statistics Reference Variable Comparison Variable .1 [3 OR p-value <50 years I. 000 50-64 years 1.090 2.974 0.063 >64 years 0.668 1.951 0.378 Spouse/live together 1. 000 Non-spouse/live together 0.435 1.545 0.394 Non-spouse/live apart 0.432 1.540 0.354 <5 0 years 1. 000 50-64 years 0.884 2.420 0.103 >64 years 0.399 1.490 0.441 Female 1. 000 Male 0156 0.856 0.671 Female 1. 000 Male 01 13 0.893 0.755 Spouse/live together 1. 000 Non-spouse/live together 0.275 1.316 0.575 Non-spouse/live apart 0.291 1.338 0.434 <5 0 years I. 000 50—64 years 1.088 2.968 0.063 >64 years 0.678 1.969 0.273 Female 1. 000 Male 0058 0.944 0.876 Spouse/live together 1. 000 Non-spouse/live together 0.427 1.532 0.406 Non-spouse/live apart 0.427 1.532 0.360 72 Table 34. Odds of disagreement for nausea Caregiver Characteristics Statistics Reference Variable Comparison Variable L [3 OR p-value <50 1. 000 50-64 0202 0.817 0.695 >64 -0.919 0.399 0.096 Spouse/live together I. 000 Non-spouse/live together 0.111 1.117 0.836 Non-spouse/live apart -0.220 0.802 0.657 <5 0 years I. 000 50-64 years 0208 0.812 0.637 >64 years -0.833 0.435 0.045 Female 1. 000 Male -0.280 0.756 0.493 Female 1.000 Male -0.390 0.677 0.328 Spouse/live together 1.000 Non-spouse/live together 0.351 1.421 0.476 Non-spouse/live apart 0.253 1.288 0.511 <50 years 1. 000 50—64 years -0.211 0.810 0.683 >64 years 0873 0.418 0.1 15 Female 1. 000 Male -0.260 0.771 0.525 Spouse/live together 1. 000 Non-spouse/live together 0.090 1.094 0.866 Non-spouse/live apart -0.239 0.787 0.630 73 Table 35. Odds of disagreement for vomiting Caregiver Characteristics Statistics Reference Variable Comparison Variable [3 OR p-value <5 0 years I. 000 50-64 years -0.996 0.370 0.265 >64 years -0.795 0.452 0.357 Spouse/live together I. 000 Non-spouse/live together 0.640 1.897 0.412 Non-spouse/live apart -0.279 0.757 0.757 <5 0 years 1. 000 50-64 years -0.938 0.391 0.234 >64 years -0.654 0.520 0.301 Female 1. 000 Male -0.346 0.707 0.608 Female 1. 000 Male -0.271 0.763 0.684 Spouse/live together I. 000 Non-spouse/live together 0.854 2.350 0.215 Non-spouse/live apart 0.118 1.126 0.862 <5 0 years I. 000 50-64 years -1.027 0.358 0.252 >64 years -0.763 0.466 0.374 Female 1. 000 Male -0.331 0.718 0.625 Spouse/live together 1. 000 Non-spouse/live together 0.605 1.831 0.435 Non-spouse/live apart -0.328 0.720 0.716 74 Table 36. Odds of disagreement for weight loss Caregiver Characteristics Statistics Reference Variable Comparison Variable [3 OR p-value <50 years I. 000 50-64 years -0.807 0.446 0.102 >64 years -0.822 0.440 0.103 Spouse/live together 1. 000 Non-spouse/live together -0.129 0.879 0.797 Non-spouse/live apart -0.300 0.741 0.523 <5 0 years 1. 000 50-64 years -0.633 0.536 0.143 >64 years 0609 0.544 0.103 Female 1. 000 Male 0.086 1.090 0.787 Female 1. 000 Male 0.050 1.051 0.874 Spouse/live together I. 000 Non-spouse/live together 0.1 13 l .1 19 0.807 Non-spouse/live apart 0.226 1.253 0.509 <5 0 years 1. 000 50-64 years 0802 0.448 0.104 >64 years -0.837 0.433 0.099 Female 1. 000 Male 0.090 1.094 0.780 Spouse/live together I. 000 Non-spouse/live together -0.1 19 0.888 0.813 Non-spouse/live apart -0.290 0.748 0.538 75 Table 37. Odds of disagreement for dry mouth Caregiver Characteristics Reference Variable Statistics Comparison Variable 0 OR p-value <5 0 years 1. 000 50-64 years 0411 0.663 0.333 >64 years 0477 0.621 0.278 Spouse/live together I. 000 Non-spouse/live together 0.109 1.115 0.799 Non-spouse/live apart 0.719 2.053 0.059 <5 0 years 1. 000 50-64 years 0684 0.505 0.069 >64 years -0.909 0.403 0.007 Female 1. 000 Male 0.030 1.030 0.915 Female 1. 000 Male 0.103 1.108 0.708 Spouse/live together 1. 000 Non-spouse/live together 0.376 1.456 0.343 Non-spouse/live apart 1.003 2.726 0.001 <5 0 years 1. 000 50-64 years -0.408 0.665 0.336 >64 years -0.487 0.615 0.270 Female 1. 000 Male 0.061 1.063 0.830 Spouse/live together 1. 000 Non-spouse/live together 0.117 1.124 0.786 Non-spouse/live apart 0.726 2.066 0.057 76 Table 38. Odds of disagreement for cough Caregiver Characteristics Statistics Reference Variable Congarison Variable [3 OR p-value <5 0 years 1. 000 50-64 years 0.276 1.317 0.575 >64 years 0007 0.993 0.989 Spouse/live together 1.000 Non-spouse/live together 0.744 2.105 0.104 Non-spouse/live apart 0.395 1.485 0.373 <5 0 years 1. 000 50-64 years -0.070 0.933 0.873 >64 years -0.421 0.656 0.291 Female 1. 000 Male -0.027 0.973 0.936 Female 1. 000 Male 0020 0.981 0.954 Spouse/live together 1.000 Non-spouse/live together 0.739 2.095 0.089 Non-spouse/live apart 0.422 1.524 0.235 <5 0 years 1. 000 50-64 years 0.276 1.318 0.574 >64 years -0.009 0.991 0.986 Female 1.000 Male 0.011 1.011 0.975 Spouse/live together I. 000 Non-spouse/live together 0.746 2.108 0.105 Non-spouse/live apart 0.397 1.487 0.374 77 Table 39. Odds of disagreement for difficulty breathing Caregiver Characteristics Statistics Reference Variable Comparison Variable [3 OR p-value <5 0 years 1. 000 50-64 years 0421 0.657 0.437 >64 years 0718 0.488 0.209 Spouse/live together 1. 000 Non-spouse/live together -1.285 0.277 0.102 Non-spouse/live apart -0.077 0.926 0.876 <5 0 years 1. 000 50-64 years -0.251 0.778 0.584 >64 years 0566 0.568 0.175 Female 1. 000 Male 0.345 1.412 0.315 Female 1. 000 Male 0.257 1.293 0.443 Spouse/live together I. 000 Non-spouse/live together -1 .014 0.363 0.178 Non-spouse/live apart 0.459 1.582 0.198 < 5 0 years 1. 000 50-64 years 0423 0.655 0.434 >64 years -0.785 0.456 0.177 Female 1. 000 Male 0.265 1.303 0.452 Spouse/live together 1. 000 Non-spouse/live together —1.270 0.281 0.109 Non-spouse/live apart -0.061 0.941 0.903 78 Table 40. Odds of disagreement for coordination problems Caregiver Characteristics Statistics Reference Variable Comparison Variable [3 OR p-value <5 0 years 1. 000 50-64 years -0.785 0.456 0.122 >64 years -1.196 0.303 0.029 Spouse/live together 1.000 Non-spouse/live together -0.285 0.752 0.649 Non-spouse/live apart 0.338 1.402 0.490 <5 0 years 1. 000 50-64 years -0.879 0.415 0.047 >64 years -1.434 0.238 0.000 Female 1. 000 Male 0.219 1.244 0.580 Female 1. 000 Male 0.140 1.150 0.716 Spouse/live together I. 000 Non-spouse/live together 0.1 72 1 .188 0.765 Non-spouse/live apart 1.052 2.865 0.004 <5 0 years I. 000 50-64 years -0.783 0.457 0.123 >64 years -1.249 0.287 0.025 Female 1. 000 Male 0.247 1.281 0.533 Spouse/live together I. 000 Non-spouse/live together -0.270 0.763 0.669 Non-spouse/live apart 0.359 1.432 0.466 79 Table 41. Odds of disagreement for frequent urination Caregiver Characteristics Statistics Reference Variable Comparison Variable .1 [3 OR p-value .. <5 0 years 1. 000 50-64 years -0.058 0.944 0.896 >64 years 0.025 1.026 0.956 Spouse/live together 1.000 Non-spouse/live together 0. 153 1.165 0.73 1 Non-spouse/live apart 0.565 1.760 0.154 <5 0 years 1. 000 50-64 years 0280 0.756 0.479 >64 years 0.245 0.783 0.488 Female 1. 000 Male 0.497 0.608 0.106 Female 1. 000 Male -0.402 0.669 0.179 Spouse/live together 1. 000 Non-spouse/live together -0.023 0.977 0.956 Non-spouse/live apart 0.427 1.533 0.157 <5 0 years 1. 000 50-64 years 0073 0.929 0.868 >64 years 0.095 1.100 0.836 Female 1. 000 Male -0.452 0.636 0.145 Spouse/live together 1. 000 Non-spouse/live together 0.096 1 .101 0.829 Non-spouse/live apart 0.520 1.682 0.191 80 Table 42. Odds of disagreement for fever Caregiver Characteristics Statistics Reference Variable Comparison Variable 0 OR p-value <5 0 years 1. 000 50-64 years 0.155 1.168 0.848 >64 years -0.809 0.445 0.356 Spouse/live together 1.000 Non-spouse/live together 0.539 1.715 0.468 Non-spouse/live apart -0.416 0.660 0.605 <5 0 years I. 000 50-64 years 0.255 1.290 0.723 >64 years -0.839 0.432 0.250 Female 1. 000 Male 0.588 1.801 0.318 Female 1.000 Male 0.357 1.429 0.526 Spouse/live together 1.000 Non-spouse/live together 0.887 2.427 0.198 Non-spouse/live apart 0.171 1.186 0.802 <5 0 years 1. 000 50-64 years 0.198 1.219 0.808 >64 years -0.931 0.394 0.300 Female 1.000 Male 0.615 1.849 0.297 Spouse/live together 1.000 Non-spouse/live together 0.598 1.819 0.432 Non-spouse/live apart -0.368 0.692 0.648 81 £58 0mm wcoEm mucoEowabw m£>= «condo 98 33% 3:30:22 mo acmEQEoU ._ Emmi 898% 8: 0.8 :8qu use 833ch M Z 895% 08 82qu can BZwBaU H m Eaten 88m 5% mo>= 83330 n E - mu Eoumq 53> m9»: 833ch n E + m0 whom emm m as m mom m _ u _ game: m 3.38 s 8 u _ a - mo 3528: ....... {gmmmfilM--.e.\.w_..mv!.ll-l- nu 8m 2 " «on a + 3 . z u m a e no 2 5823.3. ENDNOTES ENDNOTES l SEER Cancer Statistics Review, 1973-1994; American Cancer Society Cancer Facts & Figures - 1997. 2 Grossman SA, Sheidler VR, Swedeen K, Mucenski J, Piantadosi S. Correlation of Patient and Caregiver Ratings of Cancer Pain. Journal of Pain and Symptom Management 6(2):53-57 1991. 3 Curtis AE, Femsler J1. Quality of Life of Oncology Hospice Patients: A Comparison of Patient and Primary Caregiver Reports. Oncology Nursing Forum 16(1):49-53 1989. 4 Neal MB, Ingersoll-Dayton B, Starrels ME. Gender and relationship differences in caregiving patterns and consequences among employed caregivers. The Gerontologist 1997;37:804-816. 5 Ingersoll-Dayton B, Starrels ME, Dowler D. Caregiving for parents and parent-in law: is gender important? The Gerontologist 1996;36:483-491. 6 Young RF, Kahana E. Specifying caregiver outcomes: gender and relationship aspects of caregiving strain. The Gerontologist 1989;29:660-665. 7 Kramer BJ, Kipnis S. Eldercare and work-role conflict: toward an understanding of gender differences in caregiver burden. The Gerontologist 1995;35:340-348 8 Clipp EC, George LK. Patients with cancer and their spouse caregivers. Cancer 1992;69:1075-1079. 9 Dwyer J W, Coward RT. Gender, family, and long-terrn care of the elderly. In Gender, families, and eldercare. J W Dwyer & RT Coward (Eds). pp. 151-162. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. ‘0 Miller B, Cafasso L. Gender differences in caregiving: fact or artifact. The Gerontologist 1992;32:498-507. 11 Kurtz ME, Kurtz J C, Given CG, Given G. Concordance of cancer patient and caregiver symptom reports. Cancer Practice 1996;4z185-190. '2 Barusch AS, Spaid WM. Gender differences in caregiving: why do wives report greater burden? The Gerontologist 1989;29:667-676. 82 83 ‘3 Stone R, Cafferata GL, Sangl J. Caregivers of the frail elderly: a national profile. The Gerontologist 1987;27:616-626. '4 Finley NJ. Theories of family labor as applied to gender differences in caregiving for elderly parents. Journal of Marriage and the Family 1989;51:79-86. ‘5 Horowitz A. Sons and daughters as caregivers to older parents: differences in role performance and consequences. The Gerontologist 1985;25:612-617 '6 Stoller EP. Parental caregiving by adult children. Journal of Marriage and the Family 1983;45:851-858. '7 Stoller EP. Males as helpers: the role of sons, relatives, and friends. The Gerontologist 1990;30:228-235. '8 Stone R, Short P. The competing demands of employment and informal caregiving to disables elders. Medical Care 1990;28:513-526. 19 Chang C, White-Means S. The men who care: An analysis of male primary caregivers who care for frail elderly at home. The Journal of Applied Gerontology 1991 ;10:343- 358. 2° Chappell NL. Living arrangements and sources of caregiving. The Journal of Gerontology 1991 ;46:S 1 -S8. 2' Tennstedt SL, Crawford S, McKinlay JB. Determining the pattern of community care: is coresidence more important than caregiver relationship? The Journal of Gerontology 1993;48:874-883. 22 Fitting MD, Rabins PV. Men and women: do they give care differently? Generations 1985;Fall:23-26. 23 Fitting MD, Rabins PV, Lucas MJ. Caregivers for dementia patients: A comparison of men and women. Paper presented at the 37‘h Annual Scientific Meeting of the Gerontological Society of America, San Antonio. 1984. 24 Portnoy RK. Pain Management in the Older Cancer Patient. Oncology 6(2) Supplementz86-98 1992. 25 Brescia FJ, Adler D, Gray, G. Hospitalized Advanced Cancer Patients: A Profile. Journal of Pain and Symptom Management 5:222-227 1990. 26Hodgkins M, Albert D, and Daltroy L. Comparing Patients’ and their Physicians’ Assessments of Pain. Pain 23:273-277 1985. 84 27 Given BA, Given CW. Family Home Care for Cancer — A Community-Based Model. Grant #ROl NRCA 01915, funded by the National Institute for Nursing Research and the National Cancer Institute. September, l993-August, 1997. 28 Guggenmoos-Holzrnann I. How reliable are chance-corrected measures of agreement? Statistics in Medicine 12:2191-2205 1993. 29 Sheehan DC, Forman WB. Symptomatic management of the older person with cancer. Clinics in Geriatric Medicine 13:203-219 1997. 30 Dunlop GM. A study of the relative frequency and importance of gastrointestinal symptoms, and weakness in patients with far advanced cancer: student paper. Palliative Medicine 4:37-43 1989. 3 ' Irvine DM, Vincent L, Bubela N, Thompson L, Graydon J. A critical appraisal of the research literature investigating fatigue in the individual with cancer. Cancer Nursing 14:188-1991991. 32 Brown M, Carrieri V, Janson-Bjerkle S, Dodd M J. Lung cancer and dyspnea: the patient’s perception. Oncology Nursing Forum 13:19-24 1986. 33 Sarna L. Correlates of symptom distress in women with lung cancer. Cancer Practice 1(1)21-281993. BIBLIOGRAPHY BIBLIOGRAPHY Barusch AS, Spaid WM. Gender differences in caregiving: Why do wives report greater burden? The Gerontologist 1989;29:667-676. Brescia FJ, Adler D, Gray G, et a1. Hospitalized Advanced Cancer Patients: A Profile. Journal of Pain and Symptom Management 5:222-227 1990. Brown M, Carrieri V, Janson-Bjerkle S, Dodd M J. Lung cancer and dyspnea: The patient’s perception. Oncology Nursing Forum 13:19-24 1986. Chang C, White-Means S. The men who care: An analysis of male primary caregivers who care for frail elderly at home. The Journal of Applied Gerontology 1991;10:343- 358. Chappell NL. Living arrangements and sources of caregiving. The Journal of Gerontology 1991 ;46:S 1-88. Clipp EC, George LK. Patients with cancer and their spouse caregivers. Cancer 1992;69: 1075- 1 079. Curtis AE, F emsler J 1. Quality of Life of Oncology Hospice Patients: A Comparison of Patient and Primary Caregiver Reports. Oncology Nursing Forum 16(1):49-53 1989. Dunlop GM. A study of the relative frequency and importance of gastrointestinal symptoms, and weakness in patients with far advanced cancer: student paper. Palliative Medicine 4:37-43 1989. Dwyer J W, Coward RT. Gender, family, and long-terrn care of the elderly. In Gender, families, and eldercare. JW Dwyer & RT Coward (Eds). pp. 151-162. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Finley NJ. Theories of family labor as applied to gender differences in caregiving for elderly parents. Journal of Marriage and the Family 1989;51:79-86. Fitting MD, Rabins PV, Lucas MJ. Caregivers for dementia patients: A comparison of men and women. Paper presented at the 37th Annual Scientific Meeting of the Gerontological Society of America, San Antonio. 1984. 85 86 Fitting MD, Rabins PV. Men and women: do they give care difi’erently? Generations 1985;Fall:23-26. Given BA, Given CW. Family Home Care for Cancer — A Community-Based Model. Grant #ROl NRCA 01915, funded by the National Institute for Nursing Research and the National Cancer Institute. September, 1993-August, 1997. Grossman SA, Sheidler VR, Swedeen K, Mucenski J, Piantadosi S. Correlation of Patient and Caregiver Ratings of Cancer Pain. Journal of Pain and Symptom Management 6(2):53-57 1991. Guggenmoos-Holzmann I. How reliable are chance-corrected measures of agreement? Statistics in Medicine 12:2191-2205 1993. Hodgkins M, Albert D, Daltroy L. Comparing Patients’ and their Physicians’ Assessments of Pain. Pain 23:273-277 1985. Horowitz A. Sons and daughters as caregivers to older parents: differences in role performance and consequences. The Gerontologist 1985;25:612-617 Ingersoll-Dayton B, Starrels ME, Dowler D. Caregiving for parents and parent-in law: is gender important? The Gerontologist 1996;36:483-491. Irvine DM, Vincent L, Bubela N, Thompson L, Graydon J. A critical appraisal of the research literature investigating fatigue in the individual with cancer. Cancer Nursing 14:188-1991991. Kramer BJ, Kipnis S. Eldercare and work-role conflict: toward an understanding of gender differences in caregiver burden. The Gerontologist 1995;35:340—348 Kurtz ME, Kurtz JC, Given CG, Given G. Concordance of cancer patient and caregiver symptom reports. Cancer Practice 1996;4: 1 85-190. Miller B, Cafasso L. Gender differences in caregiving: fact or artifact. The Gerontologist 1992;32:498-507. Neal MB, Ingersoll-Dayton B, Starrels ME. Gender and relationship differences in caregiving patterns and consequences among employed caregivers. The Gerontologist 1997;37:804-8 1 6. Portnoy R.K. Pain Management in the Older Cancer Patient. Oncology 6(2) Supplementz86-98 1992. Sarna L. Correlates of symptom distress in women with lung cancer. Cancer Practice 1(1)21-281993. 87 SEER Cancer Statistics Review, 1973-1994; American Cancer Society Cancer Facts & Figures - 1997. Sheehan DC, Forman WB. Symptomatic management of the older person with cancer. Clinics in Geriatric Medicine 13:203-219 1997. Stoller EP. Males as helpers: the role of sons, relatives, and friends. The Gerontologist 1990;30:228-235. Stoller EP. Parental caregiving by adult children. Journal of Marriage and the Family 1983;45:851-858. Stone R, Cafferata GL, Sangl J. Caregivers of the frail elderly: a national profile. The Gerontologist 1 987;27:616-626. Stone R, Short P. The competing demands of employment and informal caregiving to disables elders. Medical Care 1990;28:513-526. Tennstedt SL, Crawford S, McKinlay J B. Determining the pattern of commmiity care: is coresidence more important than caregiver relationship? The Journal of Gerontology 1993 ;48:S74-S83. Young RF, Kahana E. Specifying caregiver outcomes: gender and relationship aspects of caregiving strain. The Gerontologist 1989;29:660-665. "111111111111111“