.._.._w MlCHIGm THEfiis a Mia'ltgifié‘late ll/I/ll/I/l/II/ll/ll/I/ll/I/ll University ’ This is to certify that the thesis entitled A REVIEW OF FACILITY DESIGN AND STUDY OF HANDLING METHODS 0N STRESS AND BEHAVIOR IN MARKET SWINE presented by I LORRIE R. BRUNDIGE has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for M.S. degree in ANIMAL SCIENCE M K24 Major professor Date J2 5/flbz BY 0-7639 MS U is an Affirmative Action/Equal Opportunity Institution PLACE IN RETURN Box to remove this checkout from your record. To AVOID FINES retum on or before date due. MAY BE RECALLED with earlier due date if requested. DATE DUE DATE DUE DATE DUE 1/98 W14 A REVIEW OF FACILITY DESIGN AND STUDY OF HANDLING METHODS ON STRESS AND BEHAVIOR IN MARKET SWINE By Lorrie R. Brundige A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University In partial fulfillment of the requirements For the degree of MASTER OF SCIENCE Department of Animal Science 1 998 ABSTRACT REVIEW OF FACILITY DESIGN AND STUDY OF HANDLING METHODS ON STRESS AND BEHAVIOR IN MARKET SWINE By Lorrie R. Brundige Ten Michigan Livestock Exchange® branches were surveyed for their loading and handling facilities. The study revealed that there are difference in design with respect to width and angle throughout MLE®’s facilities, but this may not reflect the variety of designs used in the field to load market swine. This study also revealed great variation in handling time during loading. A second study examined the use of an electric prod or a hurdle, on pig behavioral patterns and physiological responses during loading. Significant responses were observed in body temperature and heart rate between handling methods (P<.0001) and (P<.0001) respectively. Cortisol was not significant between the two handling levels (P >.2621). Observations of pigs during handling produced four behaviors between to two treatments that were significantly different. Pig: vocalization (P<.0001), Jump (P<.0001), Climb (P<.OOO1) and Fall (P<.0005) . Post handling observation revealed significant responses in two of four pig behavioral patterns. Pig: Investigate (P<.0018), Root (P<.0001), Idle (P>.1021), Step (P>.3983). ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS There are many people to thank for their patience, support and guidance in getting through this project. I feel Dr. Bob Deans deserves my most gracious and humble thanks. Vlflthout Deans “giving a girl a chance” this whole thing would not be possible. Dr. Adroaldo Zanella also deserves a much needed thank you for giving me the opportunity to study animal behavior with him. I would also like to express my appreciation to Dr. Mary Andrews, Dr. Cal Flegal, and Dr. Dale Rozeboom for your unending patience and perseverance. Al Snedegar and his staff were critical in making this project come alive at the farm. Thank you Telmo Oleas for teaching me the right and wrongs ‘in lab’ and getting me through the assays without catastrophe. Thank you Dr. Tucker and Larry Chapin for letting me use the lab to complete assays before moving back to Anthony. Thank you Jim Liesman for cradling me through the statistical analysis and your untrying patience in understanding what it was I was after. Thank you to Dr. Madona Gemus, Amy Martin, Bryan Harmison, Jill Fox, Heather Porter and students at the MSU Swine farm for the early mornings, setting up and preparing for treatment and control day sampling. Lastly, thanks Pat and Eugene Brundige for “putting me up” and “putting up with me”. iii TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES VI LIST OF FIGURES VIII INTRODUCTION SCOPE OF THESIS 1 Chapter 1 Literature Review 1.0.0 Introduction2 1.2.0 Poerualitylmplications 2 1.3.0 Welfare Implications4 1.4.0 Animal Reaction to Stimuli: Stress Defined 4 1.5.0 Pig BehavrorS 1.6.0 Short-term Behavioral and Physiological Stress Indicators ....... 11 1.7.0 EffectsofAcuteStressorsonthe Pi922 1.8.0 Interaction of Behavioral and Physiological Responses to Stimuli 30 Chapter 2 Project I: The Collection Point Study 2.0.0 Introduction32 2.1.0 Methodsand Materials.....................................................32 2.2.0 ResultsofProjectl...........................................................36 2.3.0 Discussion ofProjectl 41 2.4.0 Conclusron41 Chapter 3 Project II: Pig Response to Two Levels of Handling: Behavioral and Physiological Consequences 3.0.0 Introduction 3.1.0 Treatment Useand Identification 3.2.0 Methodsand Materials 3.3.0 Results ofProjectsll 3.4.0 DiscussionofProjectll...................................................... 3.5.0 Conclusron APPENDICES APPENDIX A Collection PointSurvey APPENDIX B Video Analysis Worksheet APPENDIX C Complete Collection Point Study Results REFERENCES .43 . ...47 .65 73 78 .81 .87 ...89 ......94 Chapter 2 Project l: 2,1 2.2 Chapter 3 Project II: 3.1 3.2 3.3 3.4 3.5 3.6 3.7 3.8 3.9 3.10 3.11 3.12 3.13 LIST OF TABLES The Collection Point Study Summary results of ramp design of 10 collection points visited Summary results of pig handling differences of8 collection points vusnted Pig Response to Two Methods of Handling: Some Behavioral and Physiological Observations Summary of environmental temperatures during during control day sampling Summary of environmental temperatures during treatment day sampling Pig group composition and treatment allocation ........... Sample collection schedule Pig behavioral patterns and definitions identified on control day Pig behavioral patterns and definitions identified on treatment day .39 4O .48 48 49 ...52 62 .63 SAS model for pig physiology: Control and treatment days ....... 64 SAS model for pig behavior: Control day 64 SAS model for pig behaviorzTreatment day 64 Statistical comparison of pig behavior: Control day ................. 68 Statistical comparison of pig behavior: Treatment day ............. 69 Statisitcal comparison of pig physiology: Control day ............... 7O Statisitcal comparison of pig physiology: Treatment day .......... 70 vi APPENDICES A.1 Complete Collection Point Study Results .. vii ...89 Chapter 2 Project I: 2.1 2.2 2.3 Chapter 3 Project II: 3.1 3.2 3.3 3.4 3.5 3.6 3.7 3.8 LIST OF FIGURES The Collection Point Study Method usedto determine angle oframp Anarrowloading ramp . .. ........... Awide and narrow loading ramp Pig Response to Two Methods of Handling: Some Behavioral and Physiological Observations Top view: Schematic of pig handling route Designated prod points on the pig Monitoring bodytemperature Saliva collection using the Scoppette Jr. Heart rate monitor adapted to pig Comparison of cortisol response: Control and treatments Comparison of heart rate response: Control and treatments Comparison of body temperature responses: Control and treatments viii .36 .37 .45 46 53 .58 .71 .72 .73 INTRODUCTION Scope of Thesis Two separate projects were completed and are presented in this thesis. Project I, “The Collection Point Study” is a field study and is a survey of Michigan Livestock Exchange® (MLE®) collection sites. Ten MLE‘” collection sites were surveyed for their handling and loading facilities. lnforrnation was gathered on the design of loading ramps and pig handling during loading. Project II, “Pig Response to Two Methods of Handling: Some Behavioral and Physiological Observations”, is a controlled experiment conducted at the Michigan State University Swine Farm and examines pig behavioral and physiological responses associated with two types of pre-slaughter handling methods. Forty-eight pigs were selected and allocated to two handling treatments. Data on behavior and physiology of the pig were collected and analyzed for the presentation. Both of the above studies are considered “methods studies” in that they are precursors to future, more comprehensive experiments on pig behavior and physiology, and their relation and causation to pig welfare and pork quality. Chapter 1 Literature Review 1.0.0 Introduction The following studies have been undertaken to more fully understand the physiological and behavioral responses of pigs during pre-slaughter handling and loading for the following reasons. First, there is a growing concern in the swine industry over economic losses from pre-slaughter handling and transportation. These include death, excess bruise trim, pale, soft, and exudative lean (PSE) and, undesirable pale to gray and watery lean. Second, there is a humanistic concern over the welfare of pigs subjected to potentially unnecessary acute stressors prior to slaughter. 1.2.0 Pork Quality Implications The pig industry is urging its producers to market more pigs via the carcass merit system. As of 1992 many processors were already paying better premiums for leaner carcasses and bigger discounts for excess carcass fat (Marbery, 1992). Traditionally, there have been costs in the form of discounts that have been absorbed by the producer. Some “of these costs include; sudden death during handling and transport, bruising, pale, soft, and exudative meat (PSE), and dark firm and dry (DFD) meat, each of which results in lower-value processed meats (Knowles et al., 1994; van Laack et al., 1993). These were estimated at more than 43 million dollars in 1994 (Pork Quality Chain, 1994). Kaufman et al., (1993) estimated that greater than twenty percent of all pork carcasses in the US. contained PSE meat. Kaufman’s survey also revealed that twenty-five percent of the pork he surveyed was either “too pale” or “too dark” (Kaufman et al., 1993). When farm animals are handled or transported, biochemical changes [especially those associated with glycogen metabolism], occur in the muscle (Hails, 1978). Pale pinkish-gray pork usually is caused by the rapid conversion of muscle glycogen to lactic acid; where rapidly falling pH and a high carcass temperature induce protein denaturation (van Laack et al., 1993; Hails, 1978). As a result, the water-binding capacity of muscle proteins declines, water leaks out of the meat (purge) and the color becomes pale and grayer (Hails, 1978) DFD meat is produced if glycogen reserves are depleted before death so that little lactic acid can be produced in the muscle after death and pH remains high (Hails, 1978). Sudden death arises from uncontrollable skeletal muscle contractions with attendant hyperrnetabolic and hypertherrnic reactions which may be triggered by handling, sexual intercourse, excessive ambient temperature and a number of chemical agents (Otsu et al., 1991). “PSE pork has many variations and is related to age, handling and environment, bacterial contamination, nutritional factors (fasting before slaughter)” and genetics (Pommier and Houde, 1993; van Laack et al., 1993). Handling and environment and other factors promote the incidence of PSE, but genetics account for sixty-percent of the problem (Kaufman et al., 1993). However, according to Pommier and Houde (1993) genotype only partially predicts the expression of PSE, and that pre-slaughter management practices were equally critical in controlling the occurrence. We have known about the porcine stress syndrome (PSS) and PSE problem since the late 1960’s (Marbery, 1992), and have not eliminated the problem because the gene has both beneficial and deleterious effects. The gene in pigs produces leaness, muscle hypertrophy resulting in a 2-3 % increase in carcass lean weight (MacLennan and Phillips, 1992). But PSS hogs are more easily excited, (Marbery, 1992) “with reactions triggered by acute stressors prior to slaughter” and devalued meat products (Maclennan and Phillips, 1992). 1.3.0 Welfare Implications Broom and Johnson (1993) define welfare as “the state of an animal as regards its attempts to cope with its environment”. According to Moberg (1985) one approach to defining the welfare of an animal is to use its stress response [behavioral and physiological] as a criteria for assessing an individual’s state of well-being. Specific criteria may include injuries to animals following handling or transport, pathology, death or the incidence of blemishes, bruised or PSE meat character, which may give us information about the welfare of the animals in the period shortly before slaughter, but the substantial genetic variation in the likelihood of PSE meat production must be taken into account (Knowles et al., 1994; Guise and Penny, 1989; Broom, 1987; Blackshaw, 1986). However, defining an animal’s ‘welI-being’ or welfare has not been precise because it inevitably incorporates subjective feelings derived from personal experiences and views of life (Moberg, 1985; Barnett and Hutson,1987) which will broaden or narrow the scope of ‘welfare’. 1.4.0 Animal Reaction to Stimuli: Stress Defined There is a growing concern about the welfare of animals, particularly food animals, a complex task that requires objective descriptive criteria to establish. Therefore, researchers have turned much of their attention to defining quantitative benchmarks for determining good, fair and poor welfare of animals. Assessing the ‘state’ of the animal, both physiologically and behaviorally, under various stimuli is complicated. One factor that contributes to the state of the animal is exposure to stress or stressors (Moberg, 1985). It is also this variable that has contributed to problems in establishing sound indicators for determining the ‘welfare’ of animals (Broom and Johnson, 1993; Barnett and Huston, 1987; Moberg, 1985). “ A thorough understanding of the concept of stress is necessary to answer this question and studies with pigs allow some physiological limits to be suggested for acceptable and unacceptable welfare” (Barnett and Hutson, 1987). Stress can be short-term (acute) or long-term (chronic). Broom and Johnson (1993) have defined stress as “an environmental effect on an individual which overtaxes its control system and reduces its fitness or appears likely to do so”. Seyle (1973) defines stress as “the biological consequence of exposure to adverse conditions”. Seyle’s concept of acute stressors is defined as lasting minutes to hours and is corticosteroid dependent (Barnett and Hutson, 1987). The physiological state of stress disappears on removal of the stress [stimulus] with no ill effects on the animal other than depletion of reserves (Barnett and Hutson, 1987). Stephens (1980) has also defined stress as “ when the rate at which stimuli are perceived by an animal deviates from the normal, or is unusually prolonged, then such stimuli can be classified as stressors”. These seemingly slight differences in definition have profound implications for how animals are reported to respond to various stressors. 1.5.0 Pig Behavior In order to determine what constitutes a behavioral ‘stress’ response, it is important to understand the pig and it’s behavior under normal conditions (McGlone, 1991; Martin and Bateson, 1986). According to Fraser and Broom (1990) there are two main categories of behavior production that take place. First, there are behaviors associated with ‘maintenance’ and include behaviors associated with ingestion, locomotion, resting, body care, thermoregulation and some social actions. More specifically, these behaviors include eating drinking, rooting, playing, exploring agonistic behaviors, comfort and eliminative behaviors, and walking (McGlone, 1991). Second, there are behaviors needed for dealing with occasional, specific and often critical circumstances (Fraser and broom, 1990). These include sexual, reproductive and matemaI/neo-natal behaviors. Specifically, sexual behaviors include the number of mounts, ejaculations, intromissions and, dismount events. Reproductive behaviors include the onset of puberty, duration of courtship and reproductive success such as pregnancy, delivery weaning and offspring. Maternal and neo-natal behaviors include periparturient behaviors including shelter seeking, nest building, actual parturition, and early neo-natal-maternal bonding (McGlone, 1991 ). 1.5.1 Physiological and Behavioral Responses to Stress Animals respond to changes or challenges in their environment by a variety of interlocking anatomical, physiological, biochemical, immunological and behavioral adaptation mechanisms (Barnett and Hutson, 1987; Ewbank, 1985). Perception of an external stimuli such as changes in temperature, a social interaction with a pen-mate, or the experience of pain, is dependent on the central nervous system (CNS) (Moberg, 1985). When a stress is perceived the CNS will (1) organize to determine what type of biological response to use, and (2) organize the stress response (Moberg, 1985). If the stimulus is perceived as threatening, three general types of biological responses are available; behavioral, autonomic, and nueroendocrine (Moberg, 1985). VWthin these biological responses there exist a patterned animal physical and [behavioral] response to stimulus and includes; (1) Changes in physical orientation to the stimulus in behavioral response include physical posture and olfactory responses (Broom and Johnson, 1993). (2) Startle response which include fight or flight, postural changes, jumps, vocalizations, intensity and is related to the extent to which the animal has been disturbed (Broom and Johnson, 1993). (3) Defensive reactions which range from growling, butting by cattle to biting or prolonged intensity making animal difficult to deflect (Broom and Johnson, 1993). However, Moberg (1985) includes another response pattern which involves conservation or with-drawl whereby the animal conserves its energy. 1.5.2 Behavioral Responses to Stress For most challenges [adverse situations] the simplest, and frequently the most economical response for the animal is to alter its behavior: either remove itself from the threat (escape) or use a displacement behavior (Stephens, 1988; Moberg, 1985). The overt expression of reduced welfare [short-term or long-term] may often take the form of abnormal behaviors (Zanella, et al., 1996; Barnett and Hutson, 1987). They define abnormal as “persistent, undesirable actions, shown by a minority of individuals in the population, which is not due to any obvious damage to the nervous system and which is not confined to the situation which elicited it” (Zanella, 1992). Under chronic stress conditions, behavior may take the form of stereotypies; morphologically similar sequences of behavior patterns with no obvious function (Schouten et al., 1991). Behaviors resulting from acute stressors are more difficult to observe and define because acute stress responses are short- terrn response and generally do not have long term detrimental consequences and thus are difficult to interpret in terms of welfare” (Barnett and Hutson, 1987). However, certain patterns of behaviors resulting from acute stressors can be categorized as follows; (1) fear-related behaviors, (2) anxiety-related behaviors, and (3) behavior due to frustration (Manser, 1993). These behaviors are extemalized behaviors based on the ‘emotions’ of the animal. Though Stephens (1988) states that the term ‘emotion’ cannot be defined but is usually comprehended to a special state of the motivation of the animal. According to Stephens (1988) fear is a predominant emotion, “it is not liked and usually produces a change in the underlying motivational state of the animal leading to modified behavior pattems”. Some causes of fear include novel stimuli such as handling and transport, matemal-infant separation and predation (Stephens, 1988). Fear responses in pigs include increases frequency of defecating and urinating, pale skin, bristled hair, trembling body, dilated eyes, nose and mouth, moderately loud vocalization, unnatural huddling together and climbing on other pigs (Buchenauer, 1981). Additionally, animals may react with immobility or freezing behavior (Broom and Johnson, 1993; Manser, 1993). The animal may also respond with an inappropriate behavior, or displacement activity, which are frequently found to be associated with evidence of reduced physiological stress (Broom and Johnson, 1993; Manser, 1993). The level of response to any given stimulus is graded and will depend on the individual animal’s situation, partly reflecting its genetic makeup and partly it’s psychological state resulting from the conditioning processes experienced at an earlier stage in life (Stephens, 1988; Levine, 1985; Moberg, 1985). And according to Blackshaw (1986) behavior patterns that are expressed need to be qualified by the type of environment or husbandry system that the behavior results in. 1.5.3 Physiological Responses to Stress Apart from the behavioral changes that an animal makes, biochemical changes also take place to aid in the maintenance of homeostasis or response to a stressor (Moberg, 1985). Actions such as increased synthesis of glucose, redirection of blood supply to certain organs, modification of digestion can help the animal cope with stress and maintain homeostasis during stress (Moberg, 1985). Fight or flight responses are characterized by the release of epinephrine and norepinephrine, an increase in heart rate and cardiac output, and acute elevation of arterial pressure and overall activity increases (Moberg, 1985). Moberg’s (1985) ‘conservation’ is characterized by an increase in adrenal-cortical activity, increase vagal activity, immobility and suppression of environmental directed activity. An animal’s perception of a stressful situation, and its behavioral and physiological modes of responding to it may be dramatically altered by experience, modifying the threat or mode of response (Moberg, 1985). This is supported by Levine (1985), who suggests that novelty and uncertainty are considered primary determinants of response and that novelty of a stimulus requires the animal to make a comparison between present stimulus events and those that have been experienced in the past. According to Moberg (1985), however, novelty does not necessarily represent a threat. Responses to stimuli vary across species and among individuals in that species and will depend on: species of animal, previous experience, nature 10 and severity of stressor and the ability to cope with it’s situation (Broom and Johnson, 1993; Manser, 1993; Stephens, 1988; Levine, 1985; Moberg, 1985). 1.6.0 Short-term Behavioral and Physiological Stress Indicators There are indices to measure “short term” or “acute” difficult situations of the animal. The use of behavioral and physiological measures reflect an integrated effect due to both external and internal stimuli imposed on an animal (Broom and Johnson, 1993). 1.6.1 Techniques for Quantifying and Evaluating Behaviors Behavior observations play a critical role in the assessment of stress (Manser, 1993). Correctly identifying behavioral responses to stress requires knowledge of the biology of the animal and knowledge of normal and abnormal behavioral expressions of the pig (McGlone, 1991; Martin and Bateson, 1986). One of the most common difficulties encountered is establishing a set of criteria which come near to defining the meaning of the term ‘stress’ (Barnett and Hutson, 1987; Stephens, 1988). Several test designs have already been developed and include open field test, preference test and conditioned emotional response tests (Stephens, 1988). Sequential analysis which is a detailed type of data summary and analysis techniques and T-maze preference tests which have been designed to study preferences for sexual partners, housing and environment to name a few. (McGlone, 1991). Although different in scope and objective all designs have several criteria for identifying, recording and analyzing behaviors. These are: (1) “ Categories are defined for each behavior observed and are generally independent of one another”. (2) Few categories will be defined in one 11 experiment due more reliability in fewer measures. (3) A detailed complete definition of each category and associated recording method would be written down before data is collected and analyzed. (4) Behaviors observed will include frequency, which is the number of occurrences of the behavior pattern per unit of time; duration which is the length of time for which a single occurrence of the behavior pattern lasts. (5) Behaviors will be identified as events or states. Events are behavior patterns of relatively short duration such as discrete body movements or vocalizations which can be approximated as points in time frequency. States are behavior patterns of relatively long duration such as prolonged activity body postures or proximity measures (McGlone, 1991; Martin and Bateson, 1986). Many behavioral responses are quantifiable, number of distress calls, frequency of kicking at a localized source of, or the duration of the response (Broom and Johnson, 1993). “It is possible to use duration and intensity of the acute stress response to address specific issues of concern by identifying the severity of difficult stressors and to determine management procedures that minimize acute stress responses” (Barnett and Hutson, 1987). 1.6.2 Physiological Measures of Stress Physiological measure of stress can be quantified. Measures such as heart rate, body temperature, pulse, respiration, blood pressure, renal blood flow, glucocorticoids, catecholamines, and beta-endorphins, to name a few, are useful measures to evaluate the short-term state of an animal (Broom and Johnson, 1993; Manser, 1993; Veum et al., 1979). Cortisol “Historically, the concept of stress has involved an emphasis on activation of the endocrine system” and includes the hypothalamus-pituitary- adrenal axis (Barnett and Hemsworth 1990; Levine, 1985). All pleasant and unpleasant stimuli, even if brief, are likely to elicit some hormonal response 12 (Broom and Johnson, 1993). Glucocorticoid (corticosterone, cortisol) levels rise in response to many short-term problems [and pleasures] in life and their measurement gives valuable information about the welfare of animals (Moberg, 1985). Adverse emotional stressors and physical challenges can cause an increase in glucocorticoid levels and include, matemal-infant separation, and aggressive encounters, thermal extremes, handling, novel environment, restraint, electric shock, steep ramps, crowding, mixing, vaccination transportation, weaning, social status, etc. (Hicks et al., 1998; Marchant, 1997, 1995; Minton, 1994; Lambooij and van Putten, 1993; Manser, 1993; Hemsworth, et al., 1989; Jesse et al., 1990; Phillips et al.,1988). However there are various other factors which can effect hormone levels such as: (1) diurnal variation, (2) sex differences, (3) specie and breed differences, and (4) early life experience (Manser, 1993; Moberg, 1985; Levine, 1985) and even copulation can produce increases in cortisol levels (Zanella, 1992). Cortisol (4-pregnene-11 [3, 17 a, 21-triol-3, 20-dione) is a naturally occurring adrenal cortical steroid produced by the adrenal cortex of the adrenal gland (Dickson, 1984). Glucocorticoids function to prepare the body for physical activity, thus there is a shift from anabolic to catabolic activity while non-essential processes are suppressed (Manser, 1993). More specifically, glucocorticoids directly stimulate the release of energy in the form of blood glucose by stimulating the liver to break down stored glycogen into glucose and indirectly through the depressions of insulin levels. 13 Glucocorticoids can also induce the release of amino acids from skeletal muscle, which are further converted by the liver into glucose, while nitrogen is excreted by the kidneys (Martin 1976). Glucocorticoids are released by the adrenal cortex in response to an emotional arousal. When an animal is exposed to external and internal stimuli, or when a source of arousal is perceived by the central nervous system, neurotransmitters in the Iimbic system stimulate the hypothalamus. Corticotrophin-releasing factor (CRF) is released by interleukin 1B, stimulating the release of adrenocorticotrophin hormone (ACTH) from the adenohypophysis (anterior pituitary gland). ACTH is transported in the blood to the adrenal cortex which in turn stimulates cortisol release from the adrenal cortex (Becker et al., 1985; Martin, 1976). Cortisol levels are checked with a feedback mechanism whereby under time of stress ACTH and CRF are produced in large amounts and thus reduce the level of cortisol that is produced (Martin, 1976). The purpose of the feedback mechanism is probably self-protective because persistent high levels of glucocorticoids would be delirious, leading to immuno—suppression and muscle wastage (Munck et al., 1984). High levels of glucocorticoids do not tend to persist in the circulation, and during chronic stress, plasma glucocorticoid levels may be normal due to the feedback mechanism (Manser, 1993). The magnitude of the adrenocortical response varies from species to species and from breed to breed and will depend on the intensity of the stimuli (Broom and Johnson, 1993). According to Broom et al. (1996) in a study comparing housing 14 conditions and transportation of pigs, found that circulating levels of cortisol returned to control (pre-treatment) levels at two hours post treatment when sampled in fifteen minute intervals. Cortisol can be measured in saliva or plasma (Cook et al., 1996; Zanella 1992; Laudet et al., 1988). However, the amount of cortisol in saliva is less than that found in plasma and requires a more sensitive test (Broom and Johnson, 1993). Plasma cortisol exist in the free and protein-bound forms whereas cortisol in saliva only exist in the free form and may be the most relevant when assessing reposes to environmental difficulties (Cook, 1996; Broom and Johnson, 1993). In pigs, studies suggest that salivary cortisol correlates with plasma cortisol (r=0.88) (Cook et al, 1996; Mendl, et al., 1991; Parrot and Mission, 1989; Parrot et al., 1989). Conversely, Blackshaw and Blackshaw (1989) found a low correlation (r =0.167) between salivary and plasma cortisol. The main difficulty in using plasma cortisol for the assessment of short- term stress deals mainly with the processing of catching and handling the animal in order to obtain a sample (Manser, 1993). The advantages of measuring cortisol in saliva are (1) it is a non-invasive technique, and (2) it exist in a ‘free’ or biologically active form, with no protein bound component as it does in plasma (Cook et al., 1996; Blackshaw, 1986). Pig cortisol levels have been have been reported in the literature. Bradshaw et al., 1996) report pre-treatment levels for 80 kg pigs as 2.475 nmol/ l. Becker et al. (1985) report 4.3 :l: .7 nmol/l. Parrot and Mission (1989) 15 report pigs in home pen to prior to treatment as 1-2 nmolll. Post-treatment or ‘stressor’ has also been reported in the literature. Measuring cortisol in plasma, Barnett and Hemsworth (1990) report significant differences in pigs handled pleasantly and unpleasantly, 0.52 nglml and 0.76 nglml (1.435 nmo/l and 2.0976 nmolll) respectively‘. Becker et al. (1985) report a significant effect on serum cortisol activity between pigs electrically stimulated verses control pigs 43.2 i .8 nglml over 14.0 1r .1 (119.232 nmolll and 38.916 nmolll, respectively). In other studies, Bradshaw et al. (1996) found no significant differences in salivary cortisol before and after loading. In a handling study completed by Gemus (1998) no significant differences were found in salivary cortisol response of pigs subjected to novice or experienced handlers. There are three cautions with the use of cortisol as a measure of stress. First, pig’s show a circadian pattern and experiments using cortisol must be sampled at the same time for repeated measure to gain meaningful information from this index (Broom and Johnson, 1993; Zanella, 1992; Becker et al., 1985). Second, cortisol levels may not accurately reflect severely stressful situations since they tend to reach maximal levels during moderate stress (Manser, 1993). Third, elevated cortisol levels indicate a change, however, it is often normal ‘adaptive’ responses (Barnett and Hemsworth, 1 990). Conversion factor from ng lml to nmolll = 2.76 16 Body Temperature Handling and transport which cause increases in adrenal cortex activity can also elevate body temperature (Manser, 1993). Many conditions are capable of causing normal variations in the body temperature of warm blooded animals including: age sex, season, time of day, environmental temperature, exercise, eating, digestion, drinking of water and disturbing events. (Broom and Johnson, 1993; Kluger, 1989; Anderson, 1984). Body temperature is regulated by peripheral, spinal, and hypothalamic thermo-receptors with normal ranges for swine ranging from 38.7-39.8 degrees Celsius (Anderson, 1984). A deep index of body temperature is most easily obtained in animals by insertion of a thermometer into the rectum (Anderson, 1984). However according to Korthals et al. (1995) and Eigenberg et al. (1995) core body temperature can be easily obtained via tympanic readings. Because rectal temperatures reach equilibrium more slowly than temperatures in many other internal sites, it is a good index of a ‘true steady state’ (Anderson, 1984). Body temperature increases in response to handling have been reported for calves (Trunkfield and Broom, 1990) and for pigs 'in response to electric shock (Veum et al. 1979). Geers et al. (1996) report significant increases in body temperature before and after transport in halothane heterozgous and homozygous compared to halothane negative pigs. Lucke et 17 There are three cautions when using body temperature as a measure of homeostasis. (1) it is important to insert the thermometer to a constant depth in each animal because a temperature gradient exists in the rectum, (2) core body temperature fluctuates diurnally, and (3) an understanding of the biology of the animal is required to identify types of responses shown when trying to asses welfare using this measure (Broom and Johnson, 1993; Anderson, 1984). Heart Rate Increases in heart rate can represent alarm and may be associated with distress (Baldock and Sibly, 1989). The sympathetic nervous system is sensitive to arousal and may be activated by handling or emotional stressors and lead to increases in heart rate (tachycardia) and blood pressure (Broom and Johnson, 1993; Manser, 1993). Functions of the cardiovascular system are largely under the control of the adrenal medullary and the autonomic nervous system with a wide range of stress events being capable of producing substantial alterations in cardiovascular function (Stephens and Perry, 1990). “Changes in heart rate and blood pressure occur rapidly and may be transitory if the stressor is not severe or persistent. (Manser, 1993). Tachycardia occurs when the level of physical activity of an animal, and hence its metabolic rate increases” (Broom and Johnson, 1993). Other factors that influence heart rate include individual characteristics, seasonality, stage of gestation, and behavior (Merchant et al., 1995,1997). Heart rate varies according to activity level but also changes when animals are preparing for 18 emergency reaction (Broom, 1987). Resting heart rate is related to body size, metabolic rate, and autonomic balance characteristics of the species and is reported to range from 70-120 beats per minute (bpm) in adult swine (Detweiler, 1984). Measurement of heart rate gives information about how much the individual is having to do to cope with the situation (Broom, 1991) provided (1) distinctions are made between metabolic changes and (2) if it can measured without causing disturbance to the animal (Broom and Johnson, 1993; Manser, 1993; Broom 1987). According Veum et al. (1979) heart rate pre-treatment levels for homozygous and heterozygous Hampshire pigs was 121.2 and 113.2 bpm’s respectively. However, in studies completed by Geers et. (1994) Stephens and Radar (1982), and Stephens and Perry (1990) reported lower resting or pre-treatment heart rates for pigs 94 :l: 3, 110, and 91 bpm's respectively. Shouten et al., (1991) showed that pig heart rate will respond to a bell sound followed by delivery of food. Marchant et al (1997) have demonstrated that heart rate will rise in response to agonistic interactions. Stephens and Radar (1982) demonstrated that heart rate will increase in response to simulated transport. Veum et al., (1979) have demonstrated that heart rate will increase in response to electrical shock. Warris et al. (1991) demonstrated that pig heart rate will increase with increasing slope of ramps. 19 1.6.3 Other Physiological Measures of Acute Stress Catecholamines Catecholamine is a term to loosely describe biologically active forms of epinephrine (adrenalin), norepinephrine (noradrenalin) and dopamine which are synthesized in the brain, sympathetic nerves and ganglia, the adrenal medulla and chromaffin cells (Martin, 1976). While the entire spectrum of epinephrine action prepares the animal for sustained physical activity, the hormone performs much broader functions (Martin, 1976). Specifically, epinephrine promotes conversion of muscle glycogen to glucose phosphate and contributes to glucagon stimulation of glycogenolysis and gluconeogenesis in the liver (Martin, 1976). Epinephrine stimulates the heart, increases cardiac output and promotes redistribution of blood supply (Martin, 1976). Epinephrine and norepinephrine together stimulate the heart, raise systolic blood pressure, decrease blood flow through the skin, mucous membranes and kidneys, increase blood flow through coronary blood vessels and dilate the bronchioles, elevate body temperature, metabolic rate and promote lipolysis (Martin, 1976). Dopamine behaves primarily as a neurotransmitter (Martin, 1976). The release of catecholamines occurs within 1-2 seconds of the perception of the initiating stimulus but their metabolism is very rapid (Martin, 1976). Blood Pressure In humans emotional stress produces the exercise pattern of cardiovascular changes and includes increased cardiac output and blood 20 pressure accompanied by increased muscle blood flow (Detweiler, 1984). Blood pressure may be obtained using systolic (SP) diastolic (DP) and mean pressure (MP according to the following formula: MP = DP+ 1/3 (SP-DP) (Manser, 1993). The most reliable method for obtaining blood pressure is an intra— arterial catheter (Manser, 1993; Detweiler, 1984). However, blood pressure can be measured indirectly and less invasively by compressing an artery and placing an inflatable cuff around the extremity. Draw backs of this method include animal habituation and unreliable results (Manser, 1993; Detweiler, 1984). Arterial blood pressure is determined by (1) the pumping action of the heart, (2) the peripheral resistance, (3) the viscosity of blood (4) the quantity of blood in the arterial system and (5) elasticity of arterial walls (Detweiler, 1984). Systolic/diastolic blood and mean pressure reported for adult swine at rest are 140/80 (mmHg) and 110 mmHg respectively (Detweiler, 1984). Veum et al., (1979) have demonstrated that blood pressure increases in response to electrical shock. Stephens and Radar (1982) have also demonstrated that blood pressure will increase in response to simulated transportation. Respiration Respiration functions to provide aveolar ventilation, panting, purring and nasal cycling. It is controlled by the respiratory center of the brainstem consisting of pneumotaxic center located rostrally in the pons, apneustic located caudally in the pons, dorsal respiratory group in the dorsal medulla 21 and the ventral respiratory group of the ventral medulla (Reece, 1984). Respiratory frequency (number of breaths per minute) of 23-27 kg (50.6-59.4 lbs.) pigs under lying conditions ranges from 32-58 cycles per minutes (Reece, 1984). Changes in respiratory rate can occur during emotional disturbances without body activity (Mellor and Murray, 1989). However, as with heart rate, changes in respiration could be a response to a situation perceived by the individual or it could merely reflect greater activity (Broom and Johnson, 1993). A unique advantage of using respiration is that it can be assessed by observation at a distance without disturbing the animal (Broom and Johnson, 1993) 1.7.0 Effects of Acute Stressors on the Pig Farm environment and management procedures can affect the behavior of pigs in variety of ways (Hunter et al., 1997). “Acute stress responses are short-term response and generally do not have long term detrimental consequences and thus are difficult to interpret in terms of welfare” (Barnett and Hutson, 1987). Situations that can lead to short term welfare problems for animals and cause changes in homeostatic physiology and abnormal behavioral expressions include human intervention by close approach, handling, certain training methods, transport, stocking density, and restraint (Stephens and Perry, 1990; Guise and Penny, 1989; Moberg, 1985). Obvious types of acute stressors reported in the literature include handling and electric shock, (Hunter et al., 1997 ; Grandin, 1989. 1980; Hemsworth and 22 Barnett, 1991; Stephens and Perry, 1990; Warris et al, 1990; Baldock and Sibly, 1989; Guise and Penny, 1989; Becker et al., 1985; Veum et al., 1979; Van Putten and Elshof, 1978) feed and water deprivation (Parrott and Mission, 1989; Houpt et al., 1983) transport (Dalin et al., 1993; McGlone et al., 1993; Nyberg et al., 1988) and vibration (Stephens and Perry, 1990). However, other environmental stressors include housing system, heat stress on food intake, body weight, physiology, and cellular immuno-function. During heat stress, social stress may also be present (Morrow-Tesch et al., 1994). Hicks et al. (1998) report that heat, cold, and shipping stressors interact with the social status of pigs to produce significant changes in behavior and had an effect on plasma cortisol, globulin, acute phase proteins, body weight and weight changes. Animals may experience acute or chronic social stress such as overt aggressive or submissive behavior (Morrow-Tesch et al., 1994). Agonistic behavior of sows was reported to influence heart rate by Marchant et al. (1995). According to Bradshaw et al. (1996) mixing pigs with other unfamiliar pigs results in marked increases in fighting behavior and travel sickness, plasma cortisol and beta-endorphins. Liptrap and Reaside (1978) showed that aggression increases plasma cortisol. 1.7.1 Effects of Pig Handling According to Hails (1978) the most stressful period in a pig’s life is during loading and unloading. During handling and loading, pigs are exposed to novel handler behavior and a novel physical environment (Grandin, 1997; Petchey et al., 1995). According to Hemsworth, (1993) the most important 23 objective during pig handling is to minimize their level of fear of close contact with humans and unfamiliar objects. In order to do this, Kiley-Worthington (1990), in writing about circus animals emphasized that “handling involves understanding the animal’s body language and being able to control one’s own, so that it does not portray certain emotions that one might be feeling, for example fear. Good handling must leave the animal with a pleasurable experience, otherwise the animal will quickly learn not to be handled.” Grandin (1997) and Moberg (1985) suggests that an animal’s reaction [fear?] will be governed by a complex interaction of genetics and previous experiences. Broom and Johnson (1993) also suggest that an animal’s reaction to stimulus, such as handling, will also depend on how the stimulus varies in time (i.e frequency and duration), intensity, (i.e. density and area), mode (i.e. visual, gustatory emotional, etc.) and degree of novelty (Grandin, 1997; Moberg, 1985). In terms of body regulation, animals show a greater response to a given stimulus if they cannot predict it, so it is treated as a novelty (Broom and Johnson, 1993). Grandin (1989) states that animals remember “more readily” painful or frightening experiences. Painful and frightening experiences can result from what Hemsworth (1993) describes as handler actions that are “aversive”. Aversive actions during handling that increase the pig’s fear of humans include hits, slaps, and kicks, electric shock (Hemsworth and Barnett, 1991). “If an unpleasant imposition was made by a human, handling may become more difficult for the human and more disturbing for the animal which is handled” (Broom and Johnson, 1993). 24 Gonyou et. al. (1986), Grandin (1988, 1987) and Petchey et al. (1995) indicated that regular handling of a positive nature resulted in an improvement in the ease of handling of pigs. Hemsworth, (1993) described some of these positive actions as pats, strokes and the hand resting on the back of the animal neck. Notwithstanding the behavior of the handler toward the pig, “there is some evidence to suggest that knowledgeable handling of pigs will improve the ease of subsequent handling and improve the welfare of pigs (Weeding et al., 1993). English (1991) defined the components of ‘stockmanship’ as appropriate knowledge, technical efficiency (skills), good judgement and observational abilities and the time available to employ these skills and abilities. In studies completed by Hemsworth et al. (1991, 1986, 1981) and Gonyou et al. (1986), results indicated that aversive handling treatments (poor stockmanship) resulted in increased levels of fear in humans, increased corticosteriod levels, and depressed growth rate in young pigs and reproductive performance in both gilts and young barrows. In a study completed by Stephens and Radar (1982) they found that handling pigs doubled heart rate. In a study completed by Robertson (1988) they found a significant association between knowledge of handling and pig death during transport. More recently Hunter et al. (1997) noted that pigs that had been handled adversely prior to the slaughter-house were more difficult to move at time of slaughter. Notwithstanding the above remarks, there are variations in behavioral responses in pigs subjected to unfamiliar handling, driving and attempts to 25 move them up a ramp and on to a vehicle (Broom and Johnson, 1993). Warris et al. (1990) suggests some of these variations may be due to important interactions between different handling methods and between handling and genotype. And according to Grandin (1997) high variability results in determining physiological stress on farm animals during handling and transport is likely to be due to great differences in stress levels due to a number of sources including handler and the environment. 1.7.2 Effects of an Electric Prod Studies show that electric shock can have physiological effects in stress-susceptible pigs (Veum et al., 1979). Other studies that indicate the use of an electric prod can have consequences on behavior and future interactions with pigs as well as damage to carcass. Hunter et al. (1997) noted that pigs that were easy to move [at the slaughter-house] were not subjected to harsh handling and electric prods. According to Hemsworth and Barnett, (1991) pigs subject to brief shocking were more fearful of humans upon close approach. In a study completed by Geverink and Lambooy (1994) of five Dutch slaughter-houses they found that rough handling (electric prod) resulted in severe skin damage of pigs at one slaughter-house. However, in a study completed by Guise and Penny (1989) the use of an electric prod had no effect on pH of the longissimus dorsi muscle value in pigs slaughtered although they found a significant interaction between skin blemishes, stocking density and use of an electric prod. However, according to Petchey et al. (1995) transport increases sexual and aggressive behaviors though the 26 previous results may be confounded with transport effects. In a study by Van Putten and Elshof (1978) they found electric prod produced an additive effect of heart rate with reactions progressively stronger with successive prod application 1.7.3 Effects of the Physical Environmental Grandin (1997) writes that pigs respond strongly to novel things, and that during handling and loading, pigs are exposed to a host of novelties in their environment. Broom and Johnson (1993) note that the novelty of the stimulus will enhance the impact of a stimulus on an animal. According to Hunter et al. (1997) it is possible to manipulate the rearing, finishing and loading environment so that pigs do not react adversely to novelty and likely be subject to harsh handling during transport and lairage. Novelties in the physical environment that can cause pigs to balk or turn around or become excited include shadows, created by uneven light distributions, extremely bright focused light sources, new or fresh bedding, reflections due to shiny surfaces, bright colors, moving or flapping objects, loud noises, and odors (Grandin, 1980, 1988,1989, 1990, 1991). Van Putten and Elshof (1978) found marked behavior differences of pigs entering unfamiliar light and darkly tunnels. Pigs have color vision (Klopfer et al., 1964). Therefore, Grandin’s (1989) suggestion of painting handling and loading facilities a uniform color may result in decreasing the pig’s response to novel bright, uneven reflective surfaces that may cause pigs to balk during handling and loading. Other 27 novelties common in the swine industry that pigs may encounter for the first time include electric livestock prods and loading ramps (Grandin, 1980, 1989, 1990, 1991; Warris et al.,1991; Hemsworth and Barnett, 1990; Guise and Penny, 1989; Hails, 1978;) 1.7.4 Effects of Ramp Design During the assembly phase, pigs are typically sorted from pens and moved into an alleyway or sorting pen that precedes the loading ramp. There is some evidence to suggest that pigs may be easily moved through wider alleys than narrower ones. Warris et al. (1991) suggested that races [alleys] of 120 cm (48 inches) over 45 cm (18 inches) greatly facilitated the movement of pigs. Many single file ramps are designed to grow narrower as they approach a plateau, acting as a squeeze chute as the pig steps into the doonlvay of the trailer. However, Grandin (1989) noted a significant decrease in ascent time on the loading ramp where pigs could file up the ramp side by side, separated only by a railing in which pigs could easily see one another. Van Putten and Elshof (1978) in a study designed to look at pigs moving through passages, observed that under difficult situations pigs like to stay together and that they prefer bodily contact and visual contact during loading. Studies have also shown that pig loading is more easily facilitated with ramp angles less than 25 degrees (Petchey et al., 1995). According to Van Putten and Elshof (1978), loading bridges [ramps] caused the most heart rate stress and was directly correlated with slope of the ramp. In their study loading ramps of 15 or 20 degrees makes loading much easier for inexperienced pigs. 28 Grandin (1997) recommends ramp angles of less than 20 degrees. Conversely, in similar studies, Warris et al. (1991) concluded that there was no difference in pig ascent time between 0 and 20 degrees. According to Petchey et al. (1995) non-slip flooring facilitates pig movement. Warris et al. (1991) found that there is a significant interaction between slope and cleat space on a loading ramp designed for pigs. Pigs were able to climb a 35 degree inclined ramp almost as well as the 20 degree ramp with narrow cleat spacing, resulting in an increase ascent time of only 13 %. They found that doubling the cleat spacing from 150 mm (5.9 inches) to 300 mm (11.8 inches) resulted in an increase ascent of the ramp by 57 %. However, Phillips et al. (1988) found that young pigs 7-8 weeks of age preferred cleat spacing of 50- 100 mm as opposed to 200-300 mm. Warris et al. (1991) found that solid ramp floors were preferable to see through or mesh floors while Grandin’s (1989) work revealed that loading was facilitated with solid walls as opposed to rails or slats that did not restrict sight distance. Phillips et al. (1988) found that young pigs preferred solid or open sides on a ramp to partially enclosed rail ramps. Pig’s have a wide angle of vision (Grandin, 1987), and this would suggest that restricting sight distance, both walls and flooring would have a significant effect on loading pigs. There are alternatives to using stationary inclined loading ramps. Hydraulic lifts and lifting decks, though physiologically most humane, are more costly and add to the already increasing costs of swine facilities (Petchey et al., 1995). Understanding how pigs respond to physical environments and to 29 handlers will result in the following benefits; 1) improved meat quality, timeliness, health and safety of personnel, and improved pig welfare (Petchey et al., 1995). 1.8.0 Interaction of Behavioral and Physiological Responses to stimuli The ability to respond behaviorally to a stressor can affect the level of physiological response (Manser, 1993). Hormonal control systems are subject to nervous influences, just as nervous control systems are affected by hormones. These two components of the response of an animal to its environment can be viewed as elements of a combined control system that determines an animal physiology and behavior (Broom and Johnson, 1993). “Animals use behavior and physiology methods in an attempt to cope with difficult conditions therefore, their measurement can allow identification of how good or poor their welfare is” (Broom, 1987). Determining when the animal is unable to cope, therefore, requires a discriminating use of behavioral and physiological indicators. There are considerable ranges that compound the problem. Not only do neuro-endocrinological, but, also the behavioral, and immunological responses to noxious stimuli extend across considerable ranges (Broom and Johnson, 1993) In any discussion on the behavior of animals in relation to stress, it is important to indicate the level (intensity) of the stress response against which the behavior is being assessed (Ewbank, 1985). To show a clear relationship between stress and behavior four criteria must be fulfilled. First, stressor (5) must be identified and ideally quantified. Second, physiological responses 30 such as epinephrine, norepinephrine or cortisol, must be quantified and ideally correlated with the stressor level and the degree of change in behavior. Third behavior change must be obvious, and, fourth, damage to the physical and or psychological well-being of the animal must be demonstrated (Ewbank, 1985). In conclusion Barnett and Hutson, (1987) states “there is an erroneous tendency to treat any acute stress response as evidence of suffering and reduced welfare. They continue with some principles to keep in mind when using both physiological and behavioral measures in determining stress responses. First they ask how much change in a physiological parameter indicates reduced welfare? And second, they warn that the sampling methods themselves may reduce the welfare of the animal. Lastly, they ask at what level of change in behavior, such as stereotypies, Ieamed helplessness, vocalizations, reduced exploratory behavior, is welfare at risk? (Barnett and Hutson, 1987). 31 Chapter 2 Project I: The Collection Point Study 2.0.0 Introduction The design of a loading facility can be critical to efficient handling and moving of pigs from pens to a livestock truck (see pages 24-27). It is well known that pigs can become excited and agitated during loading, resulting in potential injury to the handler and pig. Poor loading facility design can create animal movement awkwardness during loading, sometimes requiring the handler to use extreme measures of force to load pigs in an efficient manner. Loading facility design is commanding more attention industry wide as marketing venues change in favor of direct sale which may require producers to shore up handling and transportation protocols to accommodate rigid premium standards in the pork industry (Marbery, 1992). A pilot study was initiated with the cooperation of Michigan Livestock Exchange” (MLE°) to determine the type and use of handling and loading facilities in Michigan’s pig industry. It was believed that a representative sample of handling and loading facilities in Michigan could be observed by surveying MLEQ’s collection sites. This information could then be used to design an experimental loading system to identify how pigs negotiate and respond physiologically and behaviorally to differences in ramp design. 2.1.0 Methods and Materials A survey to gather specific data on swine loading facilities was designed. Several drafts were made of the Michigan Livestock Exchange” Collection Point Survey (SURVEY) and reviewed by selected faculty of the 32 Animal Science Department and staff from MLE° for relevance and completeness. 2.1 .1 Development of Survey The SURVEY, (see Appendix A) was drafted into five sections including: (1) collection point identification and pig flow information, (2) holding area, (3) loading area and ramp specifications, (4) shipping, and (5) additional loading ramps. Section one was designed to get general information on pig flow and type of pig coming through each facility. Section two was designed to gather information on how pigs are housed while at the facility including type and size of pen, and feed and water allocation. Section three was designed to gather specific information on the primary load out dock including; ramp construction, type and material, length, width, height, and flooring. mpg fo_ur was designed to observe shipping protocols each facility uses in order to ship pigs from collection point to processor. Section five was adapted to accommodate additional loading ramps periodically used by each facility during peak pig flow. In addition to the survey completed, photos2 and video3 were taken of handling and loading facilities, and actual loading of pigs onto a semi-truck trailer. Photos were taken for comparative-study purposes in future analysis. Video was taken in order to compare pig behavioral response in the ‘loading chute’ and would be used for the future design of the experimental loading ramp. \fideo was taken at the discretion of each branch manager. Each 2 35 millimeter camera with a flash utilizing 200 and 400 ISO film 33 video was coded and dated for future analysis of pig loading from each collection point. Following completion of video analysis, all video tapes were returned to management staff of Michigan Livestock Exchange”. 2.1.2 Ramp Measurements All loading ramps were measured using a standard tape measure. Loading ramp angles were calculated according to the following formula: Sin 0 = Opposite (height at front edge of ramp in feet) divided by hypotenuse (length of ramp floor in feet). Each ramp was measured along the hypotenuse (ramp floor) and opposite angle (front edge of ramp) and recorded. Figure 2.1 illustrates the method for obtaining the necessary measurement to calculate the angle of loading ramps in the field. All other measurements such as height of walls, etc. were measured using the same measuring device using “top to bottom” and “bottom to top” methods as necessary. Hypotenuse (ramp floor) Opposite angle , (front edge of ramp) ‘— 9 ‘ Angle determined Figure 2.1 Method used to determine angle of ramp 2.1.3 Video Taping The opportunity to videotape pigs being loaded onto to a truck occurred at eight of MLE‘D collection sites. All video taken at the collection sites was 3 JVC Super-VHS camera with light adapter and 2 hour SVHS tapes 34 reviewed for content following the completion of collection point survey visits. After contents of videos were reviewed, a video analysis worksheet was drafted (Appendix B). Each identical worksheet contained four decoding categories: (1) number of pigs loaded, (2) number of groups observed, (3) time taken to load pigs and (4) number of people loading. lnforrnation on individual pig behavioral responses during loading was not gathered. The number of pigs in each group loaded was determined by video observation (counting them as they passed through viewing area) or by asking the handler how many pigs were loaded in the group. The number of groups of pigs loaded was determined by handler entering and exiting the semi truck. Beginning handling time was established by either audio or visual closing of the gate posterior to the loading ramp. Handling time completion was established when the last pig in the group was maneuvered through the semi truck gate. The number of people loading pigs was established by counting persons helping the handler in the sorting pens, the number of people carrying a whip or prod and the number of persons on the loading ramp or entering the semi-truck. 2.1.4 Changes to Survey Some questions were removed from the survey and analysis due to the lack of relevance to the primary objective. In section one the question “what percentage of your pigs handled at this facility are “farm fresh”. In section two, “size of holding pens, construction type, and comments”. In section three the question, “on average which way does the wind blow”, was removed from the 35 analysis of survey. There were no questions removed in section four or section five and all information was gathered on loading docks. However, only the primary loading dock was considered relevant for the survey analysis and is reported here. 2.2.0 Results of the Study Ten of Michigan Livestock Exchange® collection points were surveyed. Nine of the collection points were in Michigan and the tenth in Ohio. Figures 2.2 and 2.3 are representative docks that were surveyed in the field. Table 2.1 provides specific results on the primary docks surveyed. Nine out of the ten primary docks surveyed were movable type docks. A movable dock in this case indicates that the floor of the ramp is adjustable to load trailers with more than one tier. It also indicates that the angle (elevation) of the ramp changes according to the height of the tier. No movable ramps were observed in the inclined position. Figure 2.2 A narrow loading ramp 36 Figure 2.3 Narrow (left) and wlde (right) loading ramps The base or frame material consisted of one of three types of material: wood, cement and metal. The height at the ramp front edge was consistently 1.2 meters high (4 ft), which corresponds to standardized livestock trailers that haul from 100-200 head of livestock. Dock width ranged from 0.6 meters (2 ft.) to just over 2.5 meters (8.5 ft). Estimated angles of ramps ranged from 0 to 23.5 degrees. Ramp floors varied over three types of material, cement, wood, and metal while floor design varied from a stair [cleat] type design to grooves in the case of cement flooring. The material of the walls or sides of the ramps were wood except for one, which was metal and all but two ramps had solid type walls. 37 2.2.1 Video analysis Video taken at each collection point allowed for more detailed analysis. Table 2.2 reveals the differences in pig handling over eight of ten collection points surveyed. The number of people observed to load pigs ranged from one to three persons. The minimum time taken to load pigs with one person was 23 seconds. The maximum time taken to load pigs with one person was 6 minutes and 25 seconds. The time taken to load pigs with three people ranged from 25 seconds to 4 minutes and 19 seconds. All pigs loaded were observed to be handled with a whip or a prod, or both. 2.3.0 Discussion of Project I Our initial objective in fishing MLE® collection sites was to gather information on loading ramp design and, more importantly, how pigs respond to differences in ramp design. Differences were observed in loading systems with respect to ramp width and angle. Differences in ramp width may reflect the need to utilize ramps for several species of animals. These differences can have an effect on pig behavior and physiology (see pages 24-26). These differences however, are confounded with the problems of precisely measuring the angles of the ramps. The problems associated with determining ramp angle were. (1) ramps were not consistent in construction, (2) opposite side (front edge) did not always sit at the same leVel with floor of building where ramp was located, and (3) in some cases it was a step or a hump in the middle of the ramp. An alternative method that should have been employed for measuring the slope of ramps in the field was the protractor and 38 0006.000 :0 0. 06:0 0000.30.00 5:059:00 808000005 0 0. 0.000 0.5 00.... 09006:. n . 0.000 00>. 0.00>0E-:o:v 5.05.0000 0 09006:. 0 :05 :05, ...Sw :05 :05 0060.0 ...05 :05 0060.5 :06 :9000 000>> .0005. 000.5 noo>> 000>> E0800 .000>> .000>> EOE00 .90.). .00: 0.000 0.5 0.5 0.0 0.0 0.0 0.5 0.5 0.0 . 0.0 0.0 .E. 20.0.. 2.00 2.00 2.00 2.00 2.00 0000.0 0000.0 2.00 2.00 2.00 00.000 000.5 .0005. 000.5 000.5 000.5 000.5 000.5 000.5 000.5 000.5 0020 0.05 0.05 0 0 0.00 0. 5 5 05 0.0 05 0. : .205. 0.0 0.05 0.0 5.0 0.0 0.0 0.0 0.05 0.0 5.0 .5. 5000.. 00.0 0. 5 00.0 05.0 00.0 05. 5 00.0 00.0 05.0 0.0 .E. 52.5 0.5 0. 5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 0.. 0.5 0:. 5.0.0: Doo>> RES— Uoo; Doo>> UOO>> EmEmO UOO>> Doo>> n #5800 _m_._m~mE mmmm 0.0.06.2 0 0.0065. 0306.2 0306.2 0 0.006.). 0.006.). 0.006.). 0.006.). 2000 metn. 0. .200 030 I000 I00 I500 I000 I000 I000 I000 .0...|0 IE0 00000.00 00:03 EEOQ 5:00:00 or 00 :9002 9:0. .0 0:300. >35an ...0 0.20... 39 0508.500 :0 c. 0.090. 005.000. 0500 oz :90. 000.> oz 00.0 00.0 00.0 00.0 00.0 00.0 00.0 00:05.:0 .255 .255 .255 0 0 20.5 .255 .255 .2505 .2505 5005050.). 0 0 5005355... 00:0. 005-005 050-000 000-000 0 0 000-500 550-000 050-000 000-000 000-000 0.2. 02000. 0 00-0 0 5-05 0 0 0 5-05 00-05 50-5 00-0 00-5 5 00000. 00.0 0 05 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0000.0 .0 .02 090 @5000. 0 5 0 0 0 5 5 0 5 0 0.0000 50 .02 0.05 0.05 0 0 0.00 0. 55 05 0.0 05 0. 55 .5... .2090 00.0 0.5 00.0 05.0 00.0 05.5 00.0 00.0 05.0 0.0 0... 5.52.5 m w 0. 50.00 .0l5 0.0. .0I0_.- 0 0.. N00 .0.00. _0__-0 . 0_ 00. .0...- - 0 00 _5_ 00 00:00.00 00:03 050.00 00:00:00 0 00 0005.00.20 9:05.05. 90 00 05.300. >.0EE3m «.0 0.00... 40 a plumb bob method (Doanes, 1981). This may have more precisely identified the slope in at least 8 of the primary ramps in the field. Video taken at each collection point allowed for more detailed analysis. However, upon complete review of all video, it was realized that a representative sample was not achieved. Statistical analysis was not performed on any man or pig behavioral results for the following reasons. First, limitations in the use of one camera (sight and sound) did not provide for more accurate decoding of man or pig behaviors. Second, a representative sample was not obtained for a complete an unbiased analysis. 2.4.0 Conclusion Overall, ramp systems observed in this study were very different with respect to ramp width and angle. In this survey there were considerable differences in handling reported for one to three people loading pigs. This may suggest that the handler may influence pig response during loading greater than ramp design. Difficulties were encountered in (1) precisely measuring ramp angle (2) compiling a representative sample of pig loading across different loading systems and (3) video taping the sensitive nature of pig handling in view of the public concern over animal welfare. However, this study did provide (1) the basis make preliminary observations of pig behavior response patterns in the ‘loading chute’ and (2) define behavioral patterns and aid the development of the experimental design for Project II. 41 Loading facility design industry wide will necessitate more research into the implications it will have on stress of the pig and the resulting welfare, and overall meat quality due to behavioral and physiological changes. 42 Chapter 3 Project II: Pig Response to Two Methods of Handling: Some Behavioral and Physiological Observations‘ 3.0.0 Introduction The physiological and behavioral responses that a pig undergoes during loading and pre-slaughter handling are complex and not yet easily identifiable. Measures of injury, pathology and immunological defenses are relatively precise indicators of poor welfare (Broom and Johnson, 1993). However, it is more difficult to assess stress or ‘poor welfare’ under short-term or ‘acute’ adverse conditions such as handling with an electric prod, loading up a steep ramp, and transport in a trailer that vibrates or is not well bedded. The one consideration that may play a role in improving pig welfare on the way to slaughter are the causal factors that result in poor meat quality including bruising, pale, soft and exudative (PSE) and dark firm and dry (DFD) that may result from pre-slaughter handling. The following experiment was undertaken to explore the possible relationship between pre-slaughter handling and loading and indicators of pig welfare. This study used salivary cortisol, heart rate, and body temperature along with behavioral observation to determine the physiological and behavioral consequences that result when pigs are subjected to two different handling methods. ‘ This study is one part of a two-part study which is n_ot presented here. Some methods presented here reflect the other project needs. 43 3.1-0 Treatment Use and Identification A total of two treatments were used for the handling and loading study. A treatment group consisted of three pigs. Pigs received one of two handling approaches; either method 1 (hurdle) or method 2 (prod). The treatments were designed in a nested split plot with each treatment replicated eight times over a four-week period. 3.1.1 Description of Handling Methods Handling Method 1 Hurdle (n=24) On treatment days (Tuesday or Thursday) the handler, using a hurdle5 moved three pigs out of their home pen, into the alley-way, through the access area, down the ally-way, onto the trailers, and secured them into either pen D or E7. The handler was instructed to use only hands, hurdle, and vocalization to complete loading. Figure 3.1 illustrates the route pigs traveled during this handling method. Handling Method 2 Prod (n=24) On treatment days the handler moved three pigs out of their home pen and into the access area using a hurdle. When all pigs were in this area and the handler ready, treatment application began using a livestock prod“. The electric prod (shock) was applied in thirty-second intervals on one of six designated points on the pig during a three-minute period. Figure 3.2 shows 5 A 2 ‘/2 ft x 3 1/2 foot board made of wood 6 Hart Featherweight Livestock trailers 7 Each compartment of the trailer measured 5.3 m2 and maintained a stocking density throughout monitoring period of 1.77m2 per pig 8 Sabre-Six, 5, 000 volts, 15 mA, Hot-Shot Products Co. Inc., Savage Minnesota 44 0.000 05 550.0 50: 0.390. 0530. 9:05... 90 .50 0.50E0:0m ".503 00 k 5.0 0.39.... 5 .0200 m < 0:00 E0: _ 0.0 0 .020500 I . I . I m "a mama .00...-l...-...OOOOIODOOOIIOIOO0.0......OOOOOOOOOOOOODOOOK — III||I_I_ 0. K \ e 5 . .- 0050:.E.0._. 5.20:0: u 5.30.50 3.03 05.05.01 "5.050 Irv + " 0:0... 0.0050024 u 00.50. 00. 5000. 45 Figure 3.2 Designated prod points on the pig five of the six designated points on the pig where the handler was designated to make a 'contact’. In addition to these. two points, one on each rear hind leg (hock), was designated as a contact spot. Each prod consisted of a ‘contact’ between the prod probe and the skin of the pig. Throughout the three-minute period each designated point received a ‘contact'. The standard three minutes of prod application was used prior to any pigs being loaded in this treatment. When three minutes elapsed, the handler was instructed to continue loading the pigs down the alleyway and onto the trailer where the pigs were secured into pen D or E. After the three-minute treatment application, the handler was instmcted to use as much time as needed to load the pigs. Figure 3.1 illustrate the handling route. All handling times, including the three-minute prod application. was monitored and recorded by a project staff person. Thirty-second intervals were indicated to the handler by vocalization. Handling began when the handler closed 46 indicated to the handler by vocalization. Handling began when the handler closed the gate to the access pen. Handling time ended and was marked when the treatment group was secured onto the trailer in pen D or E and when the gate closed. Pigs in both handling treatments were left on the trailer for 15 minutes in order to complete behavior and physiology sampling. 3.2.0 Methods and Materials 3.2-1 Pig Housing and Environmental Conditions All pigs were housed in the Michigan State University Grow/Finish Facility. Pigs were penned in two treatment pens. Pen A measured 8.8 m2 and Pen B measured 7.56 m2. Pigs were initially grouped according‘to the following stocking density: 6 pigs per pen with the exception of group E handling level 2 (prod), pen A, which had only five pigs due to the lack of additional pen-mates. Pen A stocking density with six pigs per pen was 1.47 m2 per pig. At five pigs per pen the stocking density equaled 1.76 m2 per pig. Pen B stocking density at six pigs per pen equaled 1.2 m2 per pig. Stocking density was within specifications used for this age of pig housed on partial or full slatted floors (PIH, 1987). Pigs were allowed ad libitum access to a standard grow/finish ration and access to water. Environmental temperatures were recorded for both Control day and Treatment day sampling and are summarized in tables 3.1 and 3.2. 47 Table 3.1 Summary of environmental temperatures during control day sampling Sampling period TM-1 TM-2 TM-3 Mean (0’) 20.5 19.9 19.6 Range (0°) 16.3-23.7 16.3-21.0 16.3-21.1 Table 3.2 Summary of environmental temperatures during treatment day sampling Sampling period TM-1 TM-2 TM-3 Mean (0°) 20.4 7.8 8.5 Range (c0) 18.6-22.6 27-159 42-159 3.2.2 Pig Selection and Use A total of forty-eight (48) pigs were used in the pig handling and loading study. Pigs were selected, pair-matched, and allocated to treatment groups (B to |) weekly based on: (1) genotype, (2) sex, (3) weight (4) pen-mate availability, and (5) littermate avoidance. The table 3.3 is a synopsis of pigs allocated to treatment groups (handling levels) used in control and treatment periods. 48 The genotype of the pigs used in this experiment consisted of purebred York, York-Landraoe cross, and Newsham stockg. All groups were evenly divided between gilts and barrows. Pigs were selected from growing and finishing pigs Table 3.3 Pig group composition and treatment allocation1o Group Sex Genotype Weight (kg) Handling Method Group B Gilts 3 Newsham 89.5 - 92.3 1 Hurdle 3 Newsham 97.3 - 123.6 2 Prod Group C Gilts 3 Newsham 1036-1264 1 Hurdle 2 Newsham 1027-1259 2 Prod 1 York-Landrace Group D Barrows 3 York 909-1209 1 Hurdle 2 York-Landrace 1045-1359 2 Prod 1 York Group E Barrows 2 Newsham 106.8-122.3 1 Hurdle 1 York-Landraoe 2 Newsham 106.4-123.2 2 Prod 1 York-Landraoe Group F Barrows 3 Newsham 120.0-145.0 1 Hurdle 3 Newsham 120.4—134.5 2 Prod Group G Gilts 3 York 1182-1236 1 Hurdle 2 Newsham 1055.4—115.9 2 Prod 1 Newsham Group H Gilts 3 York 1036-1173 1 Hurdle 2 York 121 .4-127.7 2 Prod 1 York-Landraoe Group I Barrows 3 York‘=1 102.3—116.4 1 Hurdle 3 York“ 1245-1355 2 Prod a. Pigs in group were littermates 9 Pigs used in the experiment were from the Michigan State University Swine Teaching and Research Facility. 1° Group A was a “trial-run” group and is not included in any comparison data. 49 available from the MSU Swine farm with weights ranging 90 to 145 kilograms. All pigs selected from available pigs were grouped with pen-mates. Littermates were avoided in groups B through H. In group I, pigs in each handling group were littermates. Each Thursday, pigs for both treatments were visually identified and moved into the Grow/Finish barn. At this time the pigs were weighed, marked with an easily identifiable number on the back and sides using a non-toxic, non-permanent grease paint stick11 for video analysis. Pigs were also measured for backfat using ultra-sound” and moved into treatment pens home pens13 (see figure3.1). 3.2.3 Pig Training and Acclimation Period Pigs were allowed one day to acclimate to the grow/finish barn before they were subjected to “pig training”. The training of pigs included acclimation to a simulated heart rate monitor strap, saliva collection procedure and body temperature sampling. Pig training was necessary due to the novelty of the procedures used and to deter any confounding effects these procedures could have on treatment effects (Broom and Johnson, 1993; Manser, 1993). Each treatment group (3 pigs per group) encountered a minimum of two training days prior to treatment day. Pig training time varied between treatment groups with 1' Markel LA-Co Industries Inc 1201 Pratt Blvd. Elk Grove \fillage Ill. 60007 ‘2 Pie Medical Scanner 200 VET with ASP-18 probe. Pie Medical Equipment B.V Phillipsweg, 6227 AJ Maastrict, The Netherlands. Marketed in the USA by Classic Medical Supply Inc. 19900 Mona Rd, Suite 105 Tequesta, Fl, 33469. ‘3 Backfat measurements were taken for a different part of this study and are not utilized in this study 50 each group receiving a minimum of one half-hour sampling each ‘training’ day. Pig training occurred on Friday and Saturday of each week. Pig Training Sampling Simulated heart rate monitoring straps were made out of duct tape and insulated wire. ‘Straps' were placed around the thorax of the pig immediately caudal to the forelimbs of the pig and a belt with snaps was latched to it. The straps were tightened and left on the pig throughout the duration of the ‘training’ period. Pig saliva was collected using 7.5 cm (3") cotton swabs“. Three to five swabs were inserted into the mouth of the pig, chewed on by the pig, removed, and then discarded. Each pig was sampled twice during each training period. Pigs were monitored for body temperature using an over-the-counter digital oral/rectal thermometer”. The method of sampling is as follows: the tip of the thermometer was lubricated and gently inserted into the anus of the pig. A temperature reading was indicated when the thermometer beeped or discontinued flashing (approximately one minute after insertion). No readings were recorded during “pig training”. 3.2.4 Control and Treatment Day Sampling. All pigs (n=48) were subject to physiological and behavioral monitoring for two days. Sunday and Wednesday were designated as a control days. Tuesday and Thursday were designated as treatment days. All variables (hear rate, cortisol, body temperature and behavior) were measured identically in 1‘ Q-tip®, Chesebrough-Ponds USA Co. 33 Benedict Place, Greenwhich, CT 06830, USA 51 groups C through I. Group B was not monitored for behavior due to an error in video monitoring. Sample Collectors Over the entire experiment a total of four different staff persons were used to collect samples. All sample collectors were familiarized with the sampling procedure before actual data collection was begun. 3.2.5 Samples Collected: Physiological and Behavioral Measurements Heart rate and behavior were monitored continuously through the experiment. Salivary cortisol and body temperatures were collected according to the following time line in table 3.4. Table 3.4 Sample collection schedule Pre-Handling Handling Post-Handling TM-1 TM-2 TM-3 Sample 1 2 - 3 All samples for pigs on control and treatment days were collected between 6:45 am. and 8:00 am. in the morning. Each pig was sampled three times on each of the control and treatment days for a total of six samples for each measurement collected. Sample 1 corresponds to ‘Pre-Handling’ and ‘5 B-D, Becton-Dickinson Consumers Products, Franklin Lakes, NJ 07417-1883 52 indicates that a pre-treatment sample was collected before handling treatments were applied. Sample 2 conesponds to 'Handling’ and reflects the time interval in which pigs were subjected to one of the two handling treatments. Sample 3 corresponds to ‘Post-Handling’ and reflects the fifteen-minute interval following the completion of handing in which the third and final sample of each measurement was taken. Body Temperature Pig body temperature was taken according the sampling time line. Body temperature was monitored using the process already described in the ‘pig training' section (see page 50-51). The readings were recorded onto a worksheet by project staff taking samples. Figure 3.3 shows the method by which body temperature was monitored. Figure 3.3 Monitoring body temperature 53 Salivary Cortisol Pig saliva was collected using eight-inch cotton swabs‘. Fresh pig saliva was collected in the following manner: a small group (3-4) cotton swabs were gently inserted into the pigs mouth and chewed on for approximately twenty to thirty seconds and then dismrded. The Scoppette Jr”. was then inserted into the pigs mouth, chewed on, depositing fresh saliva on the bud. Figure 3.4 shows the method in which saliva was collected from each pig using the Scoppette Jr”. Figure 3.4 Saliva collection using the Scoppette Jr. 1 Scoppette Jr®, # 34-7021-12, Birchwood Laboratories, Eden Praire, Minnesota 54 Following collection, buds were placed in 15 ml conical centrifuge tubes, capped and placed on dry ice in a styrofoam ice chest. Following completion of control or treatment day sampling and monitoring, samples were then frozen at -20 degrees Celsius until assayed. Salivary cortisol samples were analyzed using the Radio lmmuno Assay (RIA) Coat-A-Count Cortisol kit17 following specific instructions for determinations in saliva. Cortisol samples were thawed according to treatment groups (sixty-six samples) at a time. After thawing, buds were removed, a 5 cc. syringe (minus plunger) was placed into each conical tube, and buds replaced so saliva could be extracted from the cotton bud. Samples were then centrifuged at 4 degrees centigrade for 5 minutes at 1548 x 9 force. Immediately following centrifugation, cotton buds were removed from sample tubes to prevent re-absorption of saliva into cotton bud. One milliliter of saliva was then aliquoted to 1.5ml eppendorf tubes and frozen until the RIA for cortisol could performed. On the day the RIA was performed the samples were centrifuged again and 200 micro-liters was aliquoted to coat-a-count tubes in duplicate. Four plain (uncoated) 12x75mm polypropylene tubes were labeled with total counts (TC) and non-specific binding (NSB) in duplicate. The cortisol antibody coated tubes were labeled in duplicate with standards A-F and sample numbers. 25 microliters of standard A was pipetted into N38 and A tubes. Each standard and saliva sample was pipetted (200 ul) into its appropriate tube. One milliliter of [125 '] cortisol was added to each tube and vortexed. The samples were incubated at room temperature for three hours, decanted, and ‘7 Diagnostic Products Corporation 5700 West 96th Street, Los Angeles, Ca 90045-5597 55 counted using a Mark IV Gamma counter® for 1 minute. Standard curves were calculated and used to determine cortisol concentrations from saliva samples. The intra assay average percent coefficient of variations was 6.95. Heart Rate Monitoring Pig heart rate was monitored continuously during control and treatment days. Heart Rate was measured using the Polar Vantage NV Heart Rate Monitor18 consisting of a receiver (watch) and a belt (transmitter). The receiver has a memory function and stores data from the transmitter, averaging heart rate over five, fifteen or sixty seconds intervals. In this experiment, the interval was set at five-second intervals for maximum detail, which gave a total memory capacity of 11 hours. Heart rate monitors were adapted to pigs in the following manner: the receiver (watch) was protected by being inserted into flexible (durable) clear PVC tubing approximately 12.5 cm in length and 4.50 cm in diameter with holes for crimping ends once watch was activated and inserted into tubing. Insulated electric wire was laced through each end of the transmitter strap and looped at the end. A heavy elastic band with adjustable straps with snaps on both ends was latched to the wire loop. The PVC tubing with the receiver was laced onto the strap and activated. The adapted transmitter and receiver were placed on the pig in a manner described by Marchant et al. (1995, 1997). Figure 3.5 shows the manner in which heart rate monitors were placed on each pig. The transmitter was fitted around the ‘3 Polar Electro Inc., 99 Seaview Boulevard, Port Washington, NY 11050, U.S.A. 56 thorax of the pig, immediately caudal to the forelimbs. K-Y® jelly19 was substituted for electro-cardiogram gel and was applied to pig and the electrodes of the transmitter to ensure continuous transmission. Each end was duct-taped so that pigs could not easily manipulate or destroy snaps or wire. Each receiver was tested for proper functioning, set according to video monitor time, and occasionally rechecked during sampling. Each monitor was strapped onto pigs prior to the first sampling (pre-handling) of the treatment. Following the end of each treatment, heart rate monitors were removed from the pigs, turned off and information recorded down loaded into the computer using the Polar Advantage Interface System20. Data for the appropriate time period was averaged to a single number. Averages for time ‘pre-handling’ were calculated using a ten- minute period just prior to “6:55 am". Averages for ‘handling’ varied from group to group. ‘9 Ortho Pharmaceuticals Corp. Rariton, NJ 08869 2° Polar Electro Oy, Professorintie 5, 90440 Kempele Finland 57 Figure 3.5 Heart rate monitor adapted to a pig Averages were determined using the recorded handling time for each group. Handling times were matched against heart rate data print outs and averaged based on handle time. Averages for 'post-handling’ were calculated beginning four minutes after the completion of handling and averaged over a ten-minute period. 3.2.6 Behavioral Observation Pig behavior was continuously monitored by a closed circuit monitoring system° and a hand held mini-cam7. The handling of pigs was recorded using the hand held min-cam with sound capability. A designated camera-person was instructed to follow both the pigs and handler to the best of their ability through handling treatment. Once pigs were on the trailer behavior 6 Pansonic Time lapse video recorder, 2-AG-6730P, 4-Pansonic CCTV Cameras WV-BP310 and 1-Pansonic Quad Unit WJ-410 7 JVC SVHS camera with light adapter 58 monitoring continued with the use of the closed circuit monitoring system.23 One (1) 4.5 mm black and white camera was mounted in the corner of each of the pens (D and E) on the trailer. A video recorder and monitor were placed in the fonlvard compartment of the trailer. Video decoding Video from the closed monitoring system and the mini camera were decoded using the Observer software“. All cassettes were reviewed for content prior to decoding. Behavioral codes for control and treatment day; handling and post handling were developed based on initial review of video. Control Day Behavior Control day pig behavior was monitored using two cameras placed in front of pens A and B. Video was taped at 12 frames per second allowing for total recording time of six hours. Sound was not recorded due to the difficulty of adapting a microphone to the monitoring system. Following completion of sampling the video-tape was removed from recorder, labeled and modified to prevent accidental “tape ovef' of results. \fideos were checked to insure proper recording but were not viewed for content until all treatment groups had been completed. All video-tape and behavior patterns was coded by a single project staff member. Following initial review of video, behaviors and their definitions were established. Pigs in control data were observed and decoded in treatment 23 The recording system does have sound capability but due to difficulties in finding a suitable adapter, noise and vibration of the trailer it was decided that the sampler could write down any vocal responses. 2‘ Noldus Technology Systems 59 groups consisting of three pigs and observed for three periods of ten minutes equal to 600 seconds. Time period “pre-handing” behavior was coded for beginning from 6:45 am to 6:55 am. Time period ‘handling’ was established by project staff from treatment day equivalents and was identified from worksheets used on treatment days. Time period ‘post-handling’ was established fifteen minutes past the established ‘handling’. The ten-minute observation period for ‘post-handling’ began immediately following the completion of time period ‘handfingi Initial review of video revealed eleven distinct behaviors as describes in table 3.5. Of these a total of seven pig behavior patterns were statistically compared”. Behavior data discarded from statistical comparison included the categories of, “defecate”, “urinate”, “No doing” and “Pig #”. Treatment Day behavior Treatment day pig behavior was monitored using a total of five cameras. Two cameras were placed in the alleyway, and one camera each placed in the corner of pen D and E. An additional camera was used as a ‘moving camera’ operated by project staff that monitored handling. During handling the designated camera person with a mini-cam, with sound capability, was instructed to follow the pigs and handler to the best of her/his ability until pigs were secured into pens D or E. Stationary cameras recorded video-tape at 12 frames per second allowing for a total recording time of six hours. The ‘moving 25 A behavior was statistically compared if more than 50 % of pigs expressed the behavior 60 camera” recorded at 30 frames per second allowing for a total recording time of two hours. No behaviors were monitored for the “pre-handling" time period due to an oversight of project supervisor. Initial review of video revealed fifteen classes of pig behavior for all time periods and are defined in table 3.6. In time period ‘handling’ a total of four behaviors were statistically compared for ‘handling’ and included pig vocalizations, jumping away, climbing, and falling. Because handling time varied between and within treatments, observation and monitoring time varied and a statistical comparison of handling times is not included in this study. In time period ‘post-handling’ a total of four behaviors were identified and monitored for 10 minutes (600 seconds) following the completion of handling and a four minute interval and included investigative, idle, rooting the number of steps a pig took. 3.2.7 Statistical Analysis All data reported was analyzed using SAS software version 6.1226. Proc glm statements were developed for this nested split plot design. Pig is nested in treatment (rg) but not in (bt) basal or treatment day or tm (time period). Statistical analysis was generated behavior and physiology using the following models in tables 3.7-3.9. 2‘ SAS/STAT Users Guide Vol. 1 and Vol. 2. 1990 Version 6.12 4th edition. SAS Institute, SAS 1110., Cary NC., 27513 61 $655 9a on... Boat: mofiofion 9a of. 983mm 36 2 mEm E0: 95:5 2 new 9505 on“ 8 8826. m_ vac: .8306 m_ .0 8263 mm: EoEm>oE Emzcom 69:. 82> 65 c_ mgon m_ 9a or: “an; 85:23 2 flow «02 @500 02 82> Eo: .mnEzc 99a 9: oEEcEn 2 2% “oz a 9n. a: now; 53> mmmEozoE “mocha .0 $2839: 965% m_ DE 22 “co: 5 So umcosczm $292 .mmm_ ucE 2.: co Ego; £3, 35252 g :0 m_ 9a 9.55 9505 9: 9.30:9 m_ 36 .o 69.2 who .me Em co mcficfim 8: m_ 9n. 9:3 5:99? Em c_ 32.5 m-m mcxfl 9d 8ch ofiuo: c5 950:3 530:. new :55 53> .639: .995 05 am 35:68 m_ 9n. ..on>> .88 :96: on. 85 notomE 505 can Eson new; 5:5 .689 9: cm. 9655 m_ 9d condo... coEczoo ._o_>m:om >3 .2250 :o 35:62 22:5on one mEozoo 3.63.23 mi mam min... 62 39m 8:9: :2 wow. co m moxmu 9a of. 23:5 6ch m: a moon mum noes oi one doom .5 a: now... 5:5 .939: Co: .9636 m_ 9a of. 0:2 £39: 5 26.8 mm: .o ...oo: .93: co 395 E scrap 50cm 55, 5:8“ :0 m_ 9a of. mczoom EcEtmano ooze: BE macaw 9n. 3% 862.8 969 com $3.53 2:? 33:23 E9: >954“. 8308 En. o>o_2 «.222: 9a on... East: 9:65 cs 2 6:8 go one =61 mofioflon ma mi 98660 32:2on “moEfi mncflm new .8on 9a of. mummi 9mm 863 -92 Ems. < :3... 9mm 863.52 266 < 535 __mn_m 8 62m 80: g 85:. new new: 9952 .83on m_ .o EmEm>oE Emzcg m>>2m go on... “moi; 52:8 8 con 9: ho m__m>> o£ mnE=o so maEa 9a of. QEE. 23:3 .0 950:3 9a of. _moo> :oEczoo ho_>§._om >8 2253: :0 35:62 232530 one 33th 3.6323 9n. md 03m... 63 Table 3.7 SAS model for pig physiology: Control and treatment days Proc sort data=one;by bt; Proc glm data=one;class pgno rg tm; by bt; Model temp cort hr =rg pgno(rg) tm tm*rg; Test h=rg e=pgno(rg); lsmeans rglstderr e=pgno(rg); Means tm rg*tmlstderr; Run; Table 3.8 SAS model for pig behavior: Control day Proc sort data=one;by bt; Proc glm data=one;class pgno rg tm; by bt; Model pgstp pgly pgsit pgfd pgwt pginvst pgidl =rg pgno(rg) tm tm*rg; Test h=rg e=pgno(rg); lsmeans rglstderr e=pgno(rg); Means trn rg*tmlstderr; Run; Table 3.9 SAS Model for pig behavior: Treatment day Proc sort data=one;by bt; Proc glm data=one;class pgno rg tm; by bt; , Model pgvc pgjmp pgclmb pgfll pginvst pgidl pgrtpgstp =rg pgno(rg) tm tm*rg; Test h=rg e=pgno(rg); lsmeans rglstderr e=pgno(rg); Means trn rg*tmlstderr; Run; 64 3.3.0 Results of Project II Results were tabulated using Proc GLM statements. Tables 3.10, 3.11, 3.12 and 3.13 reveal the results of the statistical analysis reported for the behavior and physiological measures taken during both control and treatment days. 3.3.1 Pig Behavior Results: Control Day Table 3.10 reports the results of the statistical comparison of control day behaviors observed. Data reported reflect a ten-minute (600 sec) observation period. No behavior data is reported for group B”. There were no significant differences between any behavioral categories over the three time periods TM- 1, TM-2, or TM-3. The mean number of steps a pig took was 7.9 in TM-1, 5.8 in TM-2, and 6.7 for time period TM-3. (P>.9402). The amount of time spent lying-down was 22 % in TM-1, 15.7 % in TM-2 and 32.6 % in TM-3 (P> .9513). Pig’s spent on average, 7.9, 8.9 and, 10.1 percent of their time sitting for the three time periods respectively (P> .2173). Pigs were observed to have spent 17.4, 13.4 and, 10.4 percent in ‘maintenance’ which included feeding (P> .5885) and drinking (P>.6123) for each of the three time periods respectively. Pigs spent on average 37.0, 37.7, and 35.5 percent of their time in the investigative mode (P>.1984). Pigs spent on average 16.2, 24.1, and 19.7 percent of their time (P> .5392), respectively, in the idle mode. 27 N=42 because pigs in group B were not video-taped due to a malfunction in equipment 65 3.3.2 Pig Physiology Results Control Day Table 3.12 shows the results of the statistical comparison of physiological data gathered on control day. No significant differences in cortisol, heart rate or body temperature, were observed across the three sampling periods (P> .7883), (P>.4817) and (P>.0568) respectively. Mean cortisol was 3.64, 3.53, and 3. 42 nmolll for the three time periods respectively. Mean heart rate over the three time periods was 111.65, 118.21 and, 117.14 beats per minute. Mean body temperature over the three time periods was 39.02, 39.01, and 39.00 degrees Celsius respectively. 3.3.3 Behavior Results Treatment Day Table 3.11 shows the statistical comparison of observed behaviors on treatment day over time periods TM-1, TM-2, and TM-3. Time Period TM-1 No behavioral data was recorded for this time period2 Time Period TM-2 All classes of behavioral variables were significantly different. Mean pig number of pig vocalization for handling level 1 was .8333 and 10.33 for handling level 2 and was significantly different (P< .0001). Mean number of jumps each pig observed during handling levels 1 and 2 was zero and 5.5 respectively and was significantly different (P< .0001). The number of falls recorded for each pig during handling periods 1 and 2 was .042 and .750 and was significantly 2 Monitoring of pig behavior was not possible due to limitations in equipment capability 66 different (P< .0005). The other behavioral categories, investigative, idle, root, and steps, were not reported on in this time period3. Time Period TM-3 Mean time investigative behavior observed across handling methods 1 and 2 was 14.05 and 48.78 seconds respectively and were significantly different (P<.0018). Pigs in handling methods 1 and 2 were observed to have spent 2.3 and 8.13 percent of their time expressing this behavior. Mean time spent in the idle mode across handling methods 1 and 2 was 57.5 and 97.8 seconds and was not significantly different (P< .1021). Mean time spent rooting across handling methods 1 and 2 was 550.90 and 456.13 seconds respectively and was significantly different (P< .0001). Pigs in handling methods 1 and 2 spent 91.82 and 76.02 percent of their time expressing this behavior. 3.3.4 Physiology Results Treatment Day Cortisol Table 3.13 shows the results of the statistical comparison of physiological of the measures collected on treatments days over the three time periods TM-1, TM-2, and TM-3. Cortisol levels were not significantly different (P< .2621) between treatments over periods TM-1, TM-2, and TM-3. Mean 3 Less than 50 % of the pigs were observed to express this behavior in the time period 67 cocoa 6:003 008 oSEEéF m 55 .0323 $5 mEmmmaxo Zoom 33 6E: co c2656 65 66:9 mm_._om2mo moon... .3 Dozen 6:83 89 935?? m mezzo 3on .3258. Go LonEsc m_ bomofio £5. .m momma some om: we 32 93: me an? 6.8 we n.66. 82.0 6.9: 0.6.3 me 0.62 comm we 32 3mm we 26289862. alaoom god 5; an we N: we we mes-UN owe. me .3662, means .662 NS we use an... we cm? v.8 me Dcoon. mocmcmacfifi mtg moms m8 me new; new we QRF a? me 9955 28.0 33 one me E 6.3 we 35 39 me Does: 85.0 S we we 3 we we 6.6 as we seem 953080001— oa_c>-o elm secs. 2 orlm secs. 2 arm also: 2 636.2%... m u 5 la n E n s: >3 .2200 22223 mi ho contanEoo 303955 or.» 292. 68 oozed 9E: Em>29 9.: mezzo moi 9: ho Edema >tz c_ nozcmno 8: 92>mcom a 2m>.mcm .8 86.89 Emu oz 6 88.0 8.6 2.3 cm - o 6 am - c 6 cm m - 8.2 9.8 em - o o em - e e em F 69: 85 88.0 21cm 962. am - o 6 am - e 8 am m - E 8.03 am - o 6 am - c a em P cooroom. No no Emma 353 am - o 6 am - a 6 am m 995 89$ em - o 6 am - e e em F 8392 98.0 same mums em - o o em - o 8 am m - Kg 8.: em - o 6 am - e e em F 269695 - o 6 am 88.0 39o 83 em - c c em m - o 6 am some «3.0 em - o o em F :6“. - o 6 am 88.0 o: 21 em - e e 6.6. m - o 6 cm - - em - e. n em P 95.0 - o o cm 58.0 mm; 3 em - e o em .6. - o o em - - em - e e em F 6&2. - o 6 am 58.0 «3. 8.2 cm - c c em m - o 6 am mm; m8. em - e o em F co=e~__coo> wI:_m>d am 522 z magi ow cows. 2 magi am cows. 2 .o>fl. 339.com mus» «use Fuse >3 2059:... "9039.9. 9n ho cootooEoo 39.255 2..” 2an 69 586 $3 8.8 em Nome meme. em two 5.3 em m - 23 :.8 am 83 8.8 em 68.0 8.8 em 109939953 .68 58.0 «6.33.69 mm 8.9969 a 392.2: mm m - 8.29.8? a Roles? a and 8.2: mm P Eoeoam e8: ammo 82 8.: em was 8m 8 5m 9am 2 m . - 8.: 86 em 2.0 one 8 ohm 83 em P €35an8 o:_m>-o am 322 2 cm :35. 2 cm 23.2 2 _o>ol.r_ .933... mus: «us: was: >3 2053:. u>mo_o_o>:a 2o Co 32.353 .3335 2am 23... 88.0 83 8.8 me Bed 5.8 we 33 No.8 we recessed-ewe zoom :93 5.36;: 3 E9 as: me 8.33.5 me Eoovocnm too... 838 Ed was me «mm 93 me warm Ed 9 goes _on_too o:_m>-n_ am :35. z Um :32 2 am :35. z .2335 mus... «us: was: >3 .9530 ">mo_o_m>.._o 9a Co 39.3.63 .33sz «fin min... 70 cortisol values for handling method 1 (hurdle) and 2 (prod) were 4.08 and 3.73 nmolll for the time period TM-1. Mean cortisol values for pigs in handling methods 1 and 2 for time period TM-2 were 8.36 and 8.80 nmolll respectively. Mean cortisol values for pigs in each handling treatment in time period TM-3 was 8.50 and 11.26 nmolll respectively. Figure 3.6 shows the magnitude of cortisol response of handling methods 1 and 2 compared to that of control (basal) levels. .a. ..s O 0| _I L. AZ fl + Method 1 —I- Method 2 l I I u I — Basal Handling Period Cortisol nmolll O 0! L Figure 3.6 Comparison of cortisol response: Control and treatments Heart Rate Pig heart rate response was significantly different (P<.0001) across both handling treatments over the three rime periods. In time period TM-1 heart rate was significantly lower than recorded basal level heart rate for the same pigs. In time period TM-2, heart rate for handling method 1 rose to 1.3 times that of recorded basal levels. Heart rate in handling method 2 rose to 1.7 times that of recorded basal levels. Heart rate over both handling treatments fell in time period TM 3 to 1.03 and 1.16 times that over recorded basal levels for the same 71 time period. Figure 3.7 illustrates pig heart rate response to handling methods 1 and 2 compared to those recorded during control (basal) day. NM case‘s; \ / \ + Method 1 g + Method 2 — Basal -l Heart Rate Beats per Minute (Ave) TM=1 TM=2 TM=3 Handling Period Figure 3.7 Comparison of heart rate response: Control and treatments Body Temperature A significant difference (P< .0001) in body temperature was observed between handling methods across the three time periods TM-1, TM-2, and TM- 3. Mean body temperature (0°) for pigs in handling method 1 was 38.96, 39.09, and 39.11 for time periods TM-1, TM-2, and TM-3 respectively. Mean body temperature for pigs in handling method 2 were 38.1, 39.45 and 39.39 centigrade respectively over time periods TM-1, TM-2, and TM-3. Figure 3.8 illustrates the magnitude of response in body temperature. 72 TM=1 TM=2 TM=3 8 § 39.6 1 2"? 39.4 fi+ 1: 7; 39.2 , 5 39 _ V... +Method 1 '5 + Method 2 .5, 38.6 '— 6. 38.4 I o 1!: Handling Period Figure 3.8 Comparison of body temperature response: Control and treatments illustrates the differences between control day (basal) and differences between handling over the three time periods. 3.4.0 Discussion of Project II 3.4.1 Video Observation and Pig Behavior Control Day A total of seven behavioral patterns were observed in fifty percent or more of the pigs. All behaviors ‘decoded' during control day monitoring suggest that finishing pigs in this type of environment, a barren traditional intensive type housing system, may spend more time in ‘maintenanoe' and ‘lying’ behavioral states than other states. The videography methods employed for control day monitoring were an adequate means for observing preliminary animal behavioral patterns of pigs in this environment. These results suggest that, in this study, the selection of pigs, including genotype, sex, littermate and pen 73 mates were well balanced across the groups. Additionally, the results may indicate that pig handling and sampling, during control day, were consistent for each treatment group through all three sampling periods (Zanella,1998). 3.4.2 Treatment Day Behavior Handling TM-2 A total of four behavioral patterns were identified during handling levels 1 (hurdle) and 2 (prod) on treatment day and all were significantly different between the two handling treatments. As expected, pigs vocalized more during handling method 2 than handling method 1. However, the number of falls a pig took between handling methods were different. This may need to be investigated a little more due to the potential for increased carcass damage and poor welfare of pigs being handled under ‘farm’ or real situations utilizing an electric prod to load pigs. Climbing and jumping in this study are characteristic of ‘escape’ attempts in which pigs perceiving an adverse situation attempt to avoid or flee from it. These pigs had not been handled ‘a lot’ prior to this experiment which suggests that pigs handled with a mangate or hurdle will not attempt to climb or jump away from the handler resulting in falls or sustain other injuries. These results suggest that the handler welfare is enhanced by use of a hurdle or mangate. Though the handling methods used in this study d_ig mt produce apparent injury to the pig, use of an electric prod did produce actions which may result in injury and would be indicative of poor pig welfare. This study did not observe other behaviors that have been reported in the literature during loading. The reasons for this are three-fold: (1) there were 74 not enough cameras in place to observe the various behavioral responses. (2) the use of one moving camera-person requires focus choice on either handler or pigs, resulting in observation losses during handling. (3) cameras used for this project were not well adapted to low-lighting situations. Therefore, clarity and definition in video was poor at times resulting in unclear expressed behaviors of pigs. Overall, the results in pig behavioral response to handling suggest two things. (1) handling levels 1 (hurdle) and 2 (prod) were different enough to produce a response difference in pig behavior response. (2) However, this must be weighed against the fact that due to limitations in camera surveillance, many other potential behaviors were not observed or coded for in the video (Zanella, 1998). 3.4.2 Sampling and Pig Physiology Control Day Levels Mean cortisol, heart rate and body temperature, were not significantly different across the three time periods TM-1, TM-2, and TM-3 during control (basal) day monitoring. All basal values fell within reported ranges from other published studies of pre-treatment application. These results suggest that the methods employed for measuring these variables are valid. These results also suggest that selection of pigs, including genotype, sex, littermate and pen mate were well balanced across the groups during control period and that pig sampling for each treatment were consistent through all three sampling periods (Zanella,1998). 75 Treatment Day physiology Cortisol Salivary cortisol levels were not significantly different between handling methods over the three time periods TM-1, TM-2, and TM-3 on treatment day. Handling level 2 (prod) did cause an increase in cortisol response over handling method 1. Other studies report variable results. Parrot and Mission (1989) report significant differences in salivary cortisol response of pigs to handling and restraining. However, Bradshaw et al. (1996) studying the effects of mixing and transport report no significant differences in salivary cortisol response of 90 kg pigs after loading prior to treatment. Gemus, (1998) report no significant differences in salivary cortisol response of pigs handled. However according to Becker et. al. (1985) significant differences were observed in plasma cortisol response of pigs electrically stimulated. Lack of significant results in cortisol findings can be attributed to several factors. First, the sampling interval of fifteen minutes may have failed to give maximum response. A longer sampling interval such as 25-30 minutes may have been more appropriate (Zanella, 1998). Second, significant individual differences in pigs may have contributed to lack of cortisol response. Barnett et al. (1987), report that corticosteriod measures should be carried out in the afternoon rather than in the morning because the in-between animal variation in the hormone levels is less and more stable in plasma samples. Third, glucocorticoids can be released in response to situations that are not normally regarded as stressful and therefore, single [few] adrenal indices must be considered questionable (Broom, 1988). Fourth, 76 according the Blackshaw and Blackshaw (1989) there is a low correlation between salivary cortisol and plasma cortisol. This may indicate that though salivary cortisol levels are a sound measure of HPA activity during prolonged stress, plasma cortisol may be a more reliable measure of adrenal activity under acute stressors. Heart Rate and Body Temperature Significant treatment differences in heart rate and body temperature were observed across the three time periods. These results indicate that heart rate and rectal body temperature measures are good indicators of emotional stress and metabolic changes associated with acute stress According to the results of this study several things are indicated. This study showed an association between accelerated cardiac output and core body temperature. These physiological changes were also related to the number of vocalizations, number of escape attempt (jumps and climbs), and falls in pigs handled with an electric prod. These findings resulted in a post handling relationship in which significant differences in heart rate and core body temperature were associated with more exploratory behavior (rooting and investigative) as opposed to locomotive and non-social or participatory behavior of pigs. According to Broom and Johnson, (1993) “there is considerable diversity in the responses, both behavioral. and physiological, which animals show when disturbed. Therefore, producing a ‘uniform’ behavioral and physiological stress response may be difficult (Grandin, 1997). 77 3.5.0 Conclusion The goals of Project I and II were to develop a research and procedure capability at MSU that would form the basis for a field level study on the aggregation and transitory effects of stress and welfare on market swine as they affect loss and meat quality in the pork industry. To this extent, this study deals with the application of the existing tools of behavior science. It has been necessary to develop procedures and techniques within this system at MSU that are accurate, reliable and appropriate. These projects revealed some problems areas and also some meaningful and reliable techniques. The problem areas were: (1) the need for alternative videography methods, (2) an alternative selection strategy for experimental swine, (3) an extended time frame for cortisol evaluation and (4) the sensitivity of the field sector, both producer and marketing organizations that participate in studies involving animal welfare issues. The positive aspects of these projects were: (1) the effectiveness of the body temperature and heart rate measures as sensors of animal stress, (2) the development of techniques to handle and collect samples with market swine in gathering transitory functions, (3) the process of observing and measuring behavioral patterns in a market system and (4) the necessity to develop a method to monitor and measure field handler attitudes and techniques. It was evident in research involving stress application techniques, that it is very difficult to create a uniform, defined level of stress in market swine since animals will use various means to avoid this stress treatment resulting in great 78 variation in application effectiveness. Not only is there great variation in animal response and coping mechanisms to stessors, but there also exist great variation in handler ability to move market swine. It will be important to focus on the positive comfort zone aspects of swine handling and management in field trials if research is to be supported by the field industry. 79 APPENDIX A 80 APPENDIX A Collection Point Survey Code # C- __ Date: Collection Point: County Township Average number of pigs received: Weekly Monthly Average group size of pigs arriving from farms: Range What percentage of pigs handled at this facility are: Confined _ % Outdoor _% What percentage of your pigs handled at this facility are “Farm Fresh”? % Comments: @4423. Do pigs have free access to water? Yes D No El Do pigs have access to feed? Yes Cl No [I Variable I] If variable what percentage have free access to feed at any one time?__ % Size of holding pens m x m Capacity hd. m x m Capacity hd. m x m Capacity hd. Construction type: Floor: Wood El Cement C] Other Slatted [3 Solid 1] Sides: Wood D Metal [:1 Cement Cl Other Open sided El Solid sided El Rail Cl Height of sides: m Comments: 81 @943 How many days a week due you load pigs onto semi-trucks? Out of barns: 1 El 2 El 3 Cl 4 D 5 El 6 El Variable: At farms: 1 Cl 2 D 3 Cl 4 Cl 5 El 6 I] Variable: Number of receiving docks: 1 El 2 El 3 El 4 Cl 5 I] 6 I] Number of load-out ramps: 1 El 2 I] 3 I] 4 [3 Which direction do load-out docks face? On average which way does the wind blow? ls load-out dock protected? Yes El No D If yes, is load-out dock: Covered El Siding El Other [3 ls load-out dock curved or straight? Curved El Straight [1 Load-out dock 1 Construction type: Is load-out dock 1: Movable (up & down) El Stationary [3 Base (frame): Wood C] Metal I] Cement El Other __ Height at front edge: m. Width: m. Length: m. Angle: ° Variable El Sides: Wood [3 Metal Cl Cement El Other Solid El Slatted l] Rail El Other Height: m. Load-out dock floor: Metal El Wood El Cement D Other Floor design: Smooth D Stair El Grooved Cl Other ls bedding used on the load-out dock? Yes E] No D If yes what type: Straw D Shavings [:1 Sand C] Other [I On average how long does it take to load and fill a semi-truck? hrs. Range hrs. If you have more than 1 load-out dock, do you load more than one truck at a time? Yes I] No [:1 Variable D What type of movement enhancers are used during loading? Hurdle [3 Electric prodder El Slapper D whips El Other [I Comments: 82 See back page for additional Load-out docks Shipping Pigs What is the shortest distance traveled by pigs coming to this collection point: km ? What is the longest distance traveled by pigs coming to this collection point: km? On average, how many hours do pigs get shipped to packer from this facility? ln-state < 1 hr. El 1-2 hr. Cl 2-3 hr. I] 34 hr. [3 4-5 hr. C1 > 6 hr. El Out-of-State Range hr. Percentage of hogs transported out-of-state: % Who loads pigs on to semi-trucks? Driver only: 20% El 40% El 60% D > 80% El Staff only: 20% [I 40% El 60% D > 80% D Driver and Staff: 20% I] 40% El 60% E] > 80% El How well do semi-trucks match up to ramps: Gaps [I No gaps D If gaps, approximate difference: Vlfidth: 2cm.l] 4cm.[l 6cm.lj >80mEJ Height: 2cmEl 4cmD 60ml] >8cmD Do you cover gaps during loading? Yes [I No El What do you use to cover the gap during loading? On average, how long are pigs housed at this facility before being shipped to packer? <6hr.D 12 hr.D 18 hr.l2] 24 hr. E] 30 hr. [3 36 hr.D Longer: Range: 83 On average how long do pigs sit in the truck before leaving this facility? _hrs. ls bedding used on the semi-truck? Summer: Yes [I No D Variable I] Vlfinter: Yes El No D Variable D If yes, bedding type: Summer: Straw El Shavings [:1 Sand El Vlfinter: Straw Cl Shavings [:1 Sand El Comments: Additional load-out docks and receiving docks Date:Code # C- Load-out clock 2: Is load-out dock 2: Movable (up & down) I] Stationary D Base (frame): Wood El Metal El Cement [:1 Other _ Height at front edge: m. Width: m. Length: m. Angle: ° Variable [3 Sides: Wood Cl Metal Cl Cement El Other Solid El Slatted I] Rail [3 Other Height: m. Load-out dock floor: Metal D Wood I] Cement D Other Floor design: Smooth El Stair I] Grooved D Other Comments: Load-out dock 3 Construction type: Base (frame) : Wood [3 Metal I] Cement C] Other _ Height at front edge: m. Width: m. Length: m. Angie: ° Variable :1 Sides: Wood C! Metal I] Cement El Other Solid [3 Slatted El Rail El Other Height: m. Load-out dock floor: Metal Cl Wood El Cement I] Other 84 Floor design: Smooth D Stair El Grooved D Other Comments: 85 APPENDIX B 86 APPENDIX B Video Analysis Worksheet Collection Point: Date Analyzed: Time: Width of Ramp Length of Ramp Angle of Ramp # of Pigs Loaded # of Groups Observed Time taken to Load Number of People Loading 87 APPENDIX C 88 £92m £995 £995 392m £98m £995 £995 £996 £92m .865 oz 02 02 oz mm> 02 oz 02 oz 02 £30m 3mm>> £30m “mam £30m 539.0, .83 552 mem £30m N F m F N N N N N N m m . v m w m m m m v 0-... N o m-N m o F F v o N m- F m m m o F o o m 0 N8 ON-mF wFNF NF NF-o mF mF NDoF m To mF mo> mm> mo> mm> mm> mm> mm> me> mm> mm> mv A om mm mm om om om mm om 9 ON om- F ov-o F om om-mv mNN F N ov mm om ommN oomN oom F 000 F com F SN 80. F con F com F 00 F F 8; mm> mm> oz 02 mm> mm> mm> mm> mm> NQFFB anEN ntEN NQFFN NQmEN ...QNEN NQFFN NQOEN NQQN 59mm: awn—0 ado mmo 5&0 mac mac who who N10 FAQ 3830 .o 3996 39090.5 xooo meow. xoou c2695 9.006 50-68.. .0 a mxoou 9.368. .0 .2. egg am. So 33 289 F0 So F53 g 3.5 nmmsoc moi 2.. .295 a new. mm.< 9:20: 85:80 e\e mum anew .m>< nmzmom. moi .0 w 82> 9mm 9 .52. 838:8 3.38m >35 .50.. 5:02.00 Sea—too o x_n_zmaa< F.< 03m... 89 cozmcfiEOo EmEmofioo; m m_ xoon mi. 8806:. N xoou cab AmEgoEéocv reconfim a 3.865 F oz e2; 8.. wm> 3 gm oz .55 too>> m. F Uzom noo>> VN F 0..» mad N. F noo>> ®>O_2 02 ea; 8.. mo> E F 02 :me 7322 m. F 26m _mFmE m.m F v.0 F o. F N. F _mFmE I m a: a. 0 02 e2; 85 mo> .F. 3m 02 :me noo>> vN 26m noo>> o md mod N. F noo>> O>O_2 mac 02 9.3 no... 02 .F. N\F-F oz .85 noo>> FeN Bow noo>> o No mud N. F Doo>> O>OS_ hao oz 02 mm> e22, e2; 955 no... .8... use 8.» 02 02 5 ea .5 .F: annzmm .wsngmm ‘ mm> mm> oz .85 852.0 .55 noo>> .cmEmo noo>> em 3 e... 28 8.35 8.85 Doo>> Doo>> U00>> 98 «FF 3 we 3 we 8... RF 8.0 NF NF NF UOO>> «cm—:00 U00>> m>o_2 Fm m>o_>_ alum Q a mm> as; use mm> E F 02 ._mFm noo>> vN E—om noo>> 0d N.NF mod N. F boo>> 0>O_2 mn— oz .2; 8.. 02 .F. F szvamm wm> 850.0 8:50 ea 28 uee>> mF Qm mg N. F we m>o_>_ NQO mm> e2; 8.. 02 .F. Fem oz ._mFm _mFmE vN U__om noo>> w. F F Fd mN N. F 2.950 36.2 Fao mama exooqu..m__m..F .mocmccm EmEm>22 _Emm F cm... 922 m8: 96mg .m>< 98F ocfinmm o .oo: :0 9.68m 86% .02“. .oo: xoov 50-33 .5. 292. :9me 82w $60.33 292. .5. 56:3 .5. £22, AEV mane E0: Erma—2 mmmm xooo EmEta fig 9 .52. 5.82.8 5:53 F.< 29¢ 90 m8: 9.. .o e\e ow cm... 5.35 .33 .82 .02... 203.. o... 8.86... e 2.2.. 8.9.. 2.2. 63.98 m_ amm .8... 8.865 m 28.5 38.5 Sec 02 835255 28.5 8:955 28.5 3.265 28.5 28.5 83. mm> mm> Sec 02 mm> mm> mm> mm> mm> mm> mm> .953 8.62.5 8:255 26.5 59... oz 02 9mm. 02 oz m_nm_.m> .oz wm> m_nm_.m> mm> mm> .9285 3.03.. cc 922.5 3.... 0F 8 m m m m. S 8 v F: F: 3.8.. 8 .3 mm... F: m v 8 8 8 8 8 8 3 om 299.38 8 a... o N F-.. F F o F m; 2 NF A .95 28-6-50 m-m .3. m-m m8 m-m NF 2.. NF Fem e... .308 e885 2: 8 8: me 8 8 8 08 OF 8F 88.8 .883 me 8 mm... m m 8 3 8... F 8.0 88.8 .888 .82.... 58. 8 mag .0 u n _e _e F. n _e ..u 8 23m 83 9.. 83 22, o o mno $.25 nm.m>oo .80 II II FF. v E I F8 .50. £2; 80 glad male 80 F .8 alum minim e mu n no N ..o F .0 o. .58. 528:8 8.28. 3. can; 91 0.000 00.000. 00 0.00 02 m 0>00.0 000.00 N. F 0000 000>> F.0F 0.0 mm. F N.F 000.00 0 0>00.0 E0000 5. F 0:05 0oo>> m. F F oNF NN N.F Is 0 QJGCDCDGJCDCDOJ (D Q) .020 noe>> Fum 0:00 ueo>> mem 0.. 00.. NF 000.00 0 co a. 0 ..05 0oo>> N. F 0:05 0025 m F m4» mod N... 0oo>> 0>0.>. in IL 0 V 000601600) (1) 0>00.0 0>00.0 00.000 000.00 000>> E0800 E0000 .00600 no 0 N 0 09000 .00.”. .00.. 0.000 30000.. .E. .80: 09000 802. 820 800.000. 0.mc< . .5. 580. .8. £22, .5. 0000 .00.. .0901 8.0.0:. .080... 00.5 0.000 30000005 0x00o _000_._00< 0. .52. 580:8 8.0.8. F... 203 92 REFERENCES 93 REFERENCES Anderson, BE. 1984. Temperature Regulation and Environmental Physiology. In: Dukes Physiology of Domestic Animals (M.J. Swenson Ed.) Tenth Edition. Cornell University Press, lthica and London. Baldock, NM, and RM. Sibly. 1989. Effects of Handling and Transportation on the Heart Rate and Behavior of Sheep. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 28:15- 39. Barnett, J.L. and RH. Hemsworth. 1990. The Validity of Physiological and Behavioral Measures of Animal Welfare. Applied Animal Behavior Science. 25:177-187. Barnett, J.L., and A.G.D. Hutson. 1987. Objective Assessments of Welfare in the Pig. In: Manipulating Pig Production. APSA Committee (editors) Australian Pig Science Association. Werribee. Victoria pp. 1-22. Becker, B.A., H.F. Mayes, J.A. Nienaber, G.W. Jesse, M.E. Anderson, H. Heymann, and HE. Hedrick. 1989. Effect of Fasting and Transportation on Various Physiological Parameters and Meat Quality of Slaughter Hogs. J. Anim. Sci. 67:334. Becker,B.A., J.A. Nienaber, R.K. Christenson, R.C. Manak, J.A. DeShazer and G.L. Hahn. 1985. Peripheral Concentrations of Cortisol as an Indicator of Stress in the Pig. Am. J. Vet. Res. 46(5): 1034-1038. Becker, B.A., J.A. Nienaber, J.A. DeShazer and G.L. Hahn. 1985. Effect of Transportation on Cortisol Concentrations and on Circadian Rhythm of Cortisol in Gilts. Am. J. Vet. Res. Blackshaw, J.K. 1986. Objective Measures of Welfare in Farming Environments. Australian Vet. J. 63(1 1): 361 -364. Blackshaw, J.K, and AW. Blackshaw. 1989. Limitations of Salivary and Blood Cortisol Determinations in Pigs. Veterinary Research Communications. 13 (4): 265-271. Bradshaw, R.H., R.F. Parrott, M.L. Forsling, J.A. Goode, D.M. Lloyd, R.G. Rodway, and D.M. Broom. 1996. Stress and Travel Sickness in Pigs: Effects of Road Transport on Plasma Concentrations of Cortisol, Beta-endorphin and Lysine Vasopressin. Anim. Sci. 63: 507-516. 94 Bradshaw, R.H., R.F. Parrott, J.A. Goode, D.M. Lloyd, R.G. Rodway, D.M. and Broom. 1996. Behavioural and Hormonal Responses of Pigs During Transport: Effect of Mixing and Duration of Journey. J. Anim. Sci. 62: 547- 554. Broom, D.M., T.M. Medl, and A.J. Zanella. 1996. A Comparison of the Welfare of Sows in Different Housing Conditions. Animal Science 61: 369- 385. Broom, 0M. and K.G. Johnson. 1993. Stress and Anirpgl Welfa_r§. (Ed. D.M. Broom) Chapman and Hall London. Broom, D.M. 1987. The Veterinary Relevance of Farm Animal Ethology. Vet. Rec. 121: 400-402. Brown, S.N., T.G. Knowles, J.L. McKinstry, J.E. Edwards, M.H. Anil, and PD. Warris. 1993. Patterns of Response of Some Physiological lndices of Stress in Pigs Negotiating Loading Ramps. Anim. Prod. 56: 439 (abstract). Buchenauer, D. 1981. Parameters for Assessinng Welfare, Ethological Criteria [Swine Behavior]. Paper presented at the Seminar in the EEC Programme of Coordination of Research on Animal Welfare Brussels, November 25-26, 1980. Curr. Top. Vet. Anim. Sci. The Hague: Martinus Nijhoff. V. 11 p. 75-94. Cook, N.J., A.L. Schaeger, P. Lepage, and S. Morgan Jones. 1996. Salivary vs. Serum Cortisol for the Assessment of Adrenal Activity in Swine. (abstract) Can. J. Anim. Sci. 76(3). Dalin, A.M., U. Magnusson, J. Haggendal, and l. Nyberg. 1993. The Effect of Transport Stress on Plasma Levels of Catecholamines, Cortisol, Corticosteriod-binding Globulin, Blood Cell Count and Lymphocyte Proliferation in Pigs. Acta Veterinaria Scandinavia 34: 59-68. Detweiler, OK. 1984. Regulation of the Heart. In Dukes Physiology of Domestic Animals Tenth Edition (Ed. M.J. Swenson) Cornell University Press, lthica and London. Dickson, W.M.1984. EndocrinoIOQY, Reproduction and Lactation: in Dukes Physiology of Domestic Animals (Ed. M.J. Swenson) Tenth Edition. Cornell University Press, lthica and London. Eigenberg, R.A., G.L. Hahn, J.A. Nienaber, A.M. Parkhurst, and ME Kocher. 1995. Tympanic Temperature Decay Constants as lndices of Thermal Environments: Swine. American Society of Agricultural Engineers. 38 (4): 1203-1206. 95 English, P. V.R. Fowler, S. Baxter and B. Smith Transport, Lairage and Slaughter in “The Growing and Finishing Pig: Improving Efficiency”. 1988. Farming Press Books Alexandria, NY USA. English, PR 1991. Stockmanship, Empathy and Pig Behavior. Pig Vet. J. (26): 56-66. Ewebank, R. 1985. Behavioral Responses to Stress in Farm Animals. In: Animal Stress. American Physiological Society, Bethesda Maryland. Facts and figures for fa_rmers. (Ed. Doanes ). Copyright 1981 Doane- Westem, Inc. St. Louis, Missouri. Fraser, and D.M. Broom. “Neuroethology” 1990 in Farm Animal Behavior and Welfare (Chapter 6). Balliere Tindall, London. Geers, R., E. Bleus, T. Van Schie, H. Ville, H. Gerard, S. Janssens, G. Nackaerts, E. Decuypere, and J. Jourquin. 1994. Transport of Pigs Different with Respect to the Halothane Gene: Stress Assessment. J. Anim. Sci. 72: 2552-2558. Gemus, M. 1998. Unpublished Thesis, Michigan State University, East Lansing, MI 49885 Geverink, NA, and E. Lambooy. (1994) Treatment of Slaughter Pigs During Lairage in Relation to Behavior and Skin Damage. (abstract). Presented at the 28th International Congress of the ISAE. Research Centre Foulum, Denmark, 30-6 August 1994 Gonyou, H.W., P.H. Hemsworth, and J.L. Barnett. 1986. Effects of Frequent Interactions with Humans on Growing Pigs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 1:33-52. Grandin, T. 1980. Livestock Behavior as Related to Handling Facilities Design. Int. Stud. Anim. Prob. 1:33-52. Grandin, T. 1987. “Animal Handling”. Vet Clin. N. Amer. 3:232-338. Grandin, T. 1988. Livestock Handling Pre-slaughter. Proceedings of the 34th International Congress of Meat Science and Technology. Copenhagen, Denmark. pp. 41-45. Grandin, T. 1989. Behaviour Principles of Livestock Handling. The Professional Animal Scientist. 521-11. 96 Grandin, T. 1990. Design of Loading Facilities and Holding Pens. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 28:187-201. Grandin, T.1991. Handling Problems Caused by Excitable Pigs. Proceedings of the 37th International Congress of Meat Science and Technology. Kulmbbach, Germany. Vol. 1 pp. 249-251. Grandlin, T. 1997. Assessment of Stress During Handling and Transport. J. Anim. Sci. 75:249-257. Guise, H.J., E.J. Hunter, P.J. Baynes, P.J. Wigglesworth, H.L. Riches, and R.H.C. Penny. 1997. Observations of the Behavior of Slaughter-Weight Pigs During Transport. The Pig Journal pp. 19-29. Guise, H.J., and Penny, RH. 1989. Factors Influencing the Welfare and Carcass Quality of Pigs. Anim. Prod. 49:511-515. Hails, MR. 1978. Transport Stress in Animals: A Review. Animal Regulation Studies 12289-343. Heetkamp, M.J.W., J.W. Schrama, L. de Jong, J.W.G.M. Swinkels, W.G.P. Schouten, and MW. Bosch. 1995. Energy Metabolism in Young Pigs as Affected by Mixing. J.Anim. Sci. 73:3562-3569. Hemsworth, PH. 1993. “Behavioural Principles of Pig Handling” in Livestock Handling and Transport. (Chapter 14, pps 197-211) CAB lntemational Wallingford Oxon, UK. Hemsworth, P.H., J.L. Barnett, D. Treacy, and P. Madgwick. 1990 . The Heritability of the Trait Fear of Humans and the Association Between this Trait and Subsequent Reproductive Performance of Gilts. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 25: 85-89. Hemsworth, PH, and J.L. Barnett. 1991 The Effects of Aversively Handling Pigs either Individually or in Groups on Their Behaviour, Growth, and Corticosteroids. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 30:61-72. Hemsworth, P.H., J.L. Barnett, G.J. Coleman and C. Hanson. 1989. A Study of the Relationships Between the Attitudinal and Behavioral Profiles of Stockpersons and the Level of Fear of Humans and Reproductive Performance of Commercial Pigs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 23:245-314. Hemsowrth, PH, and J.L Barnett. 1986. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 15:55-63. Hemsworth, P.H., A. Brand, and P.J. VWIiams. 1981.Livestocck Prod. Sci. 8:67-74 97 Hicks, T.A., J.J. McGlone, C.S. Whisnant, H.G. Kattesh, and R.L. Norman. 1998. Behavioral, Endocrine, Immune and Performance Measures for Pigs Exposed to Acute Stress. J. Anim. Sci. 76:474-483. ‘ Houpt, D.A., B.A. Baldwin, T.R. Houpt, and F. Hills. 1983. Humoral and Cardiovascular Responses to Feeding in Pigs. Am. J. Physiol. 13:R279- R2884. Hunter, E.J., H.L Riches, H.J. Guise and R.H.C. Penny. 1997. The Behavior of Pigs in Lairage in Relation to Their Post-weaning Management: Results of a Postal Survey. Animal Welfare. 6:139-144. Jesse, G.W., C.N. Weiss, H.F. Mayes, and GM. Zinn. 1990. Effect of Marketing Treatments and Transportation on Feeder Pig Performance. J. Anim. Sci. 68:611-617. Kallweit, PH, 1982. Physiological Responses of Pigs to Treadmill Exercise Used as a Standardized Stress. In: Transport of Animals Intended for Breeding, Production and Slaughter (R.Moss Ed.). pp. 75-86, Martinus Nijhoff Publishers, The Hague, the Netherlands. Kaufman, R.G.W, W. Sybesma, F.J.M. Smulders, G.Eikelenboom, B. Engel, R.L.J.M. van Laack, A.H. Hoving-Bolink, P. Sterrenburg, E.V. Nordhheim, P. Walstra, and PG. van der Wal. 1993. The Effectiveness of Examining Early Postmortem Musculature to Predict Ultimate Pork Quality. Meat Sci. 34:283. Kiley-Worthington, M. 1990. Animals in Circuses and Zoos. Chiron’s World. Little Eco-Farms Publishing, 29 Delhi Rd, Pitsea, Basildon, Essex, p. 240. Kluger, M.J.1989. Body Temperature Changes During Inflammation: Their Medication and Nutritional Significance. Proceedings of the Nutrition Society. 48: 337-345. Korthals, R.L., R.A. Eigenberg, G.L. Hahn, and J.A. Nienaber. 1995. Measurement and Spectral Analysis of Tympanic Temperature Regulation in Swine. American Society of Agricultural Engineers. 38(4): 905-909. Knowles, T.G., D.H. L., Maunder and RD. Warris. 1994. Factors Affecting the Incidence of Bruising in Lambs Arriving at One Slaughter House. Vet Rec. 134244-45. Lambooij, E. and G. van Putten 1993. Transport of Pigs. in: “Livestock Handling and Transport”. CAB International Wallingford, Oxon, UK. 98 Laudat, R.M, S. Cerdas, C. Fournier, D. Guiban, B. Guilahaume, and P. Luton. 1988. Salivary Cortisol Measurement: A Practical Approach to Assess Pituitary-Adrenal Function. J. Clin. Endo. Metab. 66(2): 343-348. Levine, S. 1985. A Definition of Stress. In: Animal Stress. American Physiological Society, Bethesda Maryland. Liptrap, RM. and J.L. Raeside. 1978. Journal of Endocrinology. 76:75. Lucke, J.N., G.M. Hall, and D. Lister. 1976. Porcine Malignant Hyperthennia. I: Metabolic and Physiological Changes. Br. J. Anaesth. 48:297. MacLennan, DH. and MS. Phillips. 1992. Malignant Hypothermia. Science. 256:789-794. Manser, CE. 1993. The Assessment of Stress in Laboratory Animals. RSPCA Royal Society for the Protection of Animals. Marberry, S. 1992. “Hog Industry Grappling with PSS-gene Problem": in Feedstuffs. Decembre 21, 1992. Marchant, J.N., A.R. Rudd, D.M. Broom. 1997. The Effects of Housing on Heart Rate of Gestating Sows During Specific Behaviors. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 1232 WC pps. 1-12. Marchant, J.N., M.T. Mendl, A.R. Rudd, D.M. Broom 1995. The Effect of Agonistic Interactions on the Heart Rate of Grouped-housed Sows. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 46: 49-56. Martin, C.R. Textbook of Endocrine Physiology . New York Oxford University Press 1976. Martin, P. and P. Bateson. Measuring Behavior: an introductory guide. Chapters 3 and 4. Cambridge University Press. 1986. McGlone, J.J., J.L. Salak, E.A. Lumpkin, R.l. Nicholson, M. Gibson, and R.L. Norman. 1993. Shipping Stress and Social Status Effects on Pig Performance, Plasma Cortisol, Natural Killer Cell Activity, and Leukocyte Numbers. J. Anim. Sci. 71: 888-896. McGlone, J.J. 1991. Techniques for Evaluation and Quantification of Pig Reproductive, lngestive, and Social Behaviors. J. Anim. Sci. 69:4146-4154. 99 Mellor, DJ. and L. Murray. 1989. Effects of Tail-docking and Castration on Behavior and Plasma Concentrations in Young Lambs. Res. Vet. Sci. 46: 387-391. Mendl, M. 1991. Some Problems with the Concept of a Cut-off Point for Determining When an Animal’s Welfare is at Risk. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 31:139-146. Minton, J.E. 1994. Function of the HypothaIamic—Pituitary-Adrenal Axis and the Sympathetic Nervous System in Models of Acute Stress in Domestic Farms animals. J. Anim. Sci. 72: 1891-1898. Moberg, GP. 1985. Biological Response to Stress: Key to Assment of Animal Well-Being. In: Animal Stress (Moberg, Ed.) American Physiological Society, Bethesda Maryland. Morrow-Tesch, J.L., J.J. McGlone, and J.L. Salak-Johnson. 1994. Heat and Social Stress Effects on Pig Immune Measures. J. Anim. Sci. 72: 2599-2609. Munck, A., P.M. Guyre, N.J. Holbrook. (1984). Physiological Function of Glucocorticoids in Stress and Their Relation to Pharmacological Actions. Endocr. Rev. 5:25-44. Nyberg, L., K. Lundstrom, I. Edfors-Lilja and M. Rundgren. 1988 Effects of Transport Stress on Concentrations of Cortisol, Corticosteroid-binding Globulin and Glucocorticoid Receptors in Pigs with Different Halothane Genotypes. J. Anim. Sci. 6:1201-1211. Otsu,K. V.K. Khanna, A.L. Archibold, and D. MacLennnan. 1991. Cosegregation of Porcine Malignant Hypertherrnia and a Probable Causal Mutation in the Skeletal Muscle Ryanodine Receptor Gene in the Backcross Families. Genomics 11: 744750. Parrott, RF. and EH. Misson. 1989. Changes in Pig Salivary Cortisol in Response to Transport Simulation, Food and Water Deprivation, and Mixing. British Veterinary Journal. 145:501-505. Parrott, R.F., B.H. Misson, and BA. Baldwin. 1989. Salivary Cortisol in Pigs Following Adrenocorticotrophic Hormone Stimulation: Comparison with Plasma Levels. British Veterinary Journal. 145:362-366. Petchey, A.M., J.F. Robertson, H. Kelly, and 08. Arey. 1995. Pig Welfare for the Scottish Pig Industry Initiative. Sponsored by: Scottish Society for Prevention and Cruelty to Animals. Centre for Rural Building, SAC Aberdeen Sandra Edwards, Animal Feed Technology, SAC, Aberdeen. 100 Phillips, P.A., B.K. Thompson, and D. Fraser, 1988. Preference Tests of Ramp Designs for Young Pigs. Can. J. Anim. Sci. 68:41-48. Pommier, SA, and A. Houde. 1993 Effect of the Genotype for Malignant Hypertherrnia as Determined by a Restriction Endonuclease Assay on the Quality Characteristics of Commercial Pork Loins. J. Anim. Sci. 71:420. Pork Industry Handbook, 1987. Extension Bulletin E-1283. Cooperative Extension Service, Michigan State University, East Lansing, Michigan, 48824. Reece, W.O. 1984. Respiration in Mammals: in Dukes Physiology of Domestic Animals (Ed M.J. Swenson) Tenth Edition. Cornell University Press, lthica and London. Robertson, J.F. 1988. Bacon Pigs: A Practical Approach to the Reduction of Death in Transit. In: A. M. Petchey (Ed.) “New Perspectives in Pig Production”. NOSCA, Aberdeen. Seyle, H. 1973. The Evolution of the Stress Concept. Amer. Scient. 61: 692- 699. Schouten, W., J.Rushen ,A.M.B De Passille. 1991. Stereotypic Behavior and Heart Rate in Pigs. Phys. and Behav. 50: 617-624. Stephens, DB. and GO Perry. 1990. The Effects of Restraint, Handling, Simulated and Real Transport in the Pig (with reference to man and other species). Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 28:41-55. Stephens, DB. 1988. A Review of Experimental Approaches to the Analysis of Emotional Behaviour and Their Relation to Stress in Farm Animals. Cornell Vet. 78: 1 55-1 77. Stephens, DB, and RD. Radar. 1982. The Effects of Simulated Transport and Handling on Heart Rate, Blood Pressure and Renal Arterial Blood Flow in the Pig. (abstract) Appl. Anim. Ethol. 82409-410. Stephens, DB. 1980. Stress and Its Measurement in Domestic Animals: A Review of Behavioural and Physiological Studies Under Field and Laboratory Situations. Advanced Veterinary Science Comp. Med. 24:179-210. Trunkfield, HR. and D.M. Broom. 1990. The Welfare of Calves During Handling and Transport. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 28: 135-152. 101 van Laack, R.L.J.M, C. Faustman, and JG. Sebraneck. 1993. Pork Quality and the Expression of Stress Protein Hsp 70 in Swine. J.Anim. Sci. 71: 2958-2964. Van Putten, G., and W.J. Elshof. 1978. Observations on the Effect of Transport on the Well-Being and Lean Quality of Slaughter Pigs. Anim. Reg. Stud. 1:247-271. Veum, T.L.,.R. Ellersieck, T.L. Durhham, W.R. McVickers, S.N. Williams, and J.F. Lasely. 1979. Response of Stress-Susceptible and Stress-Resistant Hampshire Pigs to Electrical Stress. I. Physiological and Biochemical effects. J. Anim. Sci. (3) 48:446-458. Warris, P.D. , S.N. Brown and T.G. Knowles. 1991. Effect of the Angle of Slope on the Ease with which Pigs Negotiate Loading Ramps. Vet. Rec. 128:419-421. Warris, P.D., S.N. Brown, E.A. Bevis, and SC. Kestin. 1990. The Influence of Pre-Slaughter Transport and Lairage on Meat Quality in Pigs of Two Genotypes. Animal Production. 50:165. Weeding, C.M., E.J. Hunter, H.J. Guise, and R.H.C. Penny. 1993. The Effect of Ease of Handling on the Welfare of Slaughter Pigs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 38:79 (Abstract). Zanella, A.J. 1992. Sow welfare indicators and their inter-relationship. Ph.D. thesis, University of Cambridge. Zanella, A.J. 1998. Personal Communication. Zanella, A.J., D.M. Broom, J.C. Hunter, and T. Mendl. 1996. Brain Opioid Receptors in Relation to Stereotypies, Inactivity, and Housing in Sows. Phys. and Behav. 59 (4/5): 769-775. 102 MICHIGRN STATE UNIV. LIBRARIES llIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIlllllIllIIIIIIIIIUIIIIIIIIHI 31293017197389