5:5. wwrdghfiflrflfizgfi tho-h.“ 11-15823 “BRA“ “'°"'°‘“\\\\\\\\\\\\\\\\\\\~\\ Michigan sum i\\\\\\\\\\\\\\\\\\\\\ . University 3 1293 This is to certify that the dissertation entitled "YEAH BUT I THOUGHT IT WOULD STILL MAKE A SQUARE": A STUDY OF FOURTH-GRADERS' DISAGREEMENT DURING WHOLE-GROUP MATHEMATICS DISCUSSION presented by Carol J. Crumbaugh has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for Ph . D . degree in Counseling , Educational Psychology and Special Education WM Major professor Date 8’15,” MS U is an Affirmative Action/Equal Opportunity Institution 0-12771 PLACE IN RETURN Box to remove this checkout from your record. TO AVOID FINES return on or before date due. MAY BE RECALLED with earlier due date if requested. DATE DUE DATE DUE DATE DUE 1!” W14 ”YEAH BUT I THOUGHT IT WOULD STILL MAKE A SQUARE”: A STUDY OF FOURTH-GRADERS’ DISAGREEMENT DURING WHOLE-GROUP MATHEMATICS DISCUSSION By Carol]. Crumbaugh A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment Of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Counseling, Educational Psychology, and Special Education 1998 ABSTRACT "YEAH BUT I THOUGHT IT WOULD STILL MAKE A SQUARE”: A STUDY OF BOURTHGRADERS' DISAGREEMENT DURING WHOLE-GROUP MATHEMATICS DISCUSSION By Carol]. Crumbaugh The purpose of this exploratory study was to examine student disagreement during whole-group discussion in one fourth grade mathematics class. Data collection drew on qualitative method. The focal data source for this study Of fourth-graders’ disagreements were transcripts from each Of the ten lessons which comprised the data set. Additional data sources included students’ math journals, two interviews with the teacher, and two interviews each with seven randomly-selected students. The data were triangulated and analyzed to determine (a) what disagreement was in this fourth grade classroom, (b) what students needed to learn to be involved in disagreements, (c) the role Of this teacher and these students during disagreements, and (d) the relationship between disagreements and mathematical tasks. The findings of this study revealed several discussion-related patterns. Discussion within which disagreement was situated unfolded in an ebb and flow to reveal a discourse pattern that this teacher established for whole—group discussion. In this pattern, mathematical ideas spiraled forward as participants jointly constructed meaning. The function Of disagreement shed light on its mathematical significance. Whereas some disagreements functioned to point out a surface flaw in a representation, others functioned to force children tO get at the underpinnings Of another’s position, or to hold up two ideas for public scrutiny. The relationship between purpose, function, and mathematical significance Of the disagreements led to the examination Of mathematical tasks that fueled thinking. Two competencies, social and academic, constituted mathematical disagreement. Both were necessary for children to mathematically disagree. The mathematical significance of the disagreements was connected to the teacher’s role. Disagreement varied in its influence on the discourse. When the mathematical and interactional aspects Of students’ disagreements were tightly woven, the mathematical disagreement appeared to contribute to the discussion and propel it forward. By contrast, when the disagreement was laden by the interactional aspect, the disagreement created a gap in the discussion. This study contributes to emerging research on the role Of discourse in mathematics teaching and learning and suggests that children need to learn more than mathematics to be involved in mathematical disagreement. Copyright by CAROL JEAN CRUMBAUGH 1998 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS It is difficult to find the words to adequately express my appreciation to everyone who helped in the completion of this dissertation. TO my committee I extend my gratitude for steadfast support and encouragement through the dissertation process. AS my dissertation director and mentor, Deborah Loewenberg Ball fostered and sustained my growth as a researcher and writer. Deborah is an exemplary role model, and I am grateful for her wisdom and thoughtfulness. Ralph Putnam, my guidance committee director, generously gave Of his time to meet, read draft documents, and generally provide support along the way. Thank you. Penelope Peterson, Sandra Wilcox, and Helen Featherstone nudged my thinking and writing with insightful questions. My cohort and colleagues, Troy Mariage, Lauren Pfeiffer, Garnet Hauger, and Lynn Brice steadfastly provided support beyond measure. Our many conversations continue to influence my thinking about research, learning, and teaching. Kara Suzuka, Tammy Lantz, and Cathy Siebert lent special support in the final stages Of writing. Thank you. This study would not be complete without heartfelt thanks to Katherine Vandenberg. Her decision to study her mathematics teaching so early in her professional development is indicative of her dedication to students and to teaching. In addition, I salute Pamela Schram and all members of Mathematics Study Group for all they do on behalf Of children. TO my family and home-town friends, I embrace you for your love and support from beginning to end. ii TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES - -- - - , - vi LIST OF FIGURES _- , , , - - - - -- _ ....... vii CHAPTER ONE: INTRODUCTION TO THE STUDY -- ...... -- -- - - _ -_ - - -1 CHAPTER TWO LITERATURE REVIEW ........... - - _ ...... ........ - --6 Theoretical Framework ................................................................................................ 6 Analytical Framework ............................................................................................... 10 Participant Structure ....................................................................................... 11 Classroom Discourse .................................................................................................. 14 Mathematical Discourse ............................................................................................ 21 Curriculum ................................................................................................................. 25 Argument and Disagreement .................................................................................... 27 Summary and Conclusion ......................................................................................... 32 CHAPTER THREE METHOD - - - ........... - ............. ............ - 34 Site, Setting, and Participants .................................................................................... 38 Method ............................................................................................................. 39 Analysis ....................................................................................................................... 46 Use of Hypermedia in Analysis ..................................................................... 49 iii CHAPTER FOUR INSIDE DISAGREEMENT _ 55 Lesson Format ............................................................................................................ 55 Discourse Pattern ....................................................................................................... 57 Teacher Moves ............................................................................................................ 61 Asking Questions ............................................................................................ 62 Connecting Ideas and Getting Children to Say More .................................. 62 Behavior and Task Reminders ....................................................................... 62 Typology Of Discussion ............................................................................................. 63 Repetitive Discussions .................................................................................... 64 Discussions With Different Ideas, Without Disagreement ......................... 65 Discussions With Different Ideas, Teacher-Mediated Disagreement ......... 67 Student-to—Student Disagreement ................................................................. 68 What is Disagreement? .............................................................................................. 70 Categories of Disagreement ...................................................................................... 74 CHAPTER FIVE SINGLE DISAGREEMENT - - - -- - 77 Correct, Revise, or Mediate ....................................................................................... 81 Defend ......................................................................................................................... 85 Kim’s Diamond ............................................................................................... 97 Call for Clarification, Question Another’s Assumptions ...................................... 102 Disagreement Over the Location Of 1 / 2 on the Number Line .................. 114 Monica’s Square: Five Pieces or Four, and Are They Equal? ................... 117 Summary ................................................................................................................... 121 CHAPTER SIX EPISODES OF DISAGREEMENT..-- - - A- - - 128 May 3 ......................................................................................................................... 126 What is the Disagreement About? ............................................................... 140 The Teacher’s Role ........................................................................................ 146 May 7 ......................................................................................................................... 153 Math on May 7 .............................................................................................. 154 What is the Disagreement About? ............................................................... 168 iv CHAPTER SEVEN CONCLUSIONS AND IMPLICATIONS -- -- - ........ 178 Summary and Discussion ........................................................................................ 178 Theory and Practice .................................................................................................. 185 Theory ............................................................................................................ 185 Practice ........................................................................................................... 188 Limitations ................................................................................................................ 189 Questions for Future Study ..................................................................................... 190 APPENDIX A - ..... - 194 APPENDIX B - - - - 196 APPENDIX C 198 APPENDIX D-_ _ _- . - _ _ _ ............................ - . 201 APPENDIX E - _ ...... 203 BIBLIOGRAPHY - - - - - -- 216 LIST OF TABLES Table 1: Purposes Of Disagreement Grouped by Function .................................... 80 Table 2: Participant Rights and Duties .................................................................. 165 Table 3: Student Pronoun Use on May 3 and May 7 ............................................ 174 vi LIST OF FIGURES Figure 1: Michaela's Way to Make Half on May 7 .................................................. 61 Figure 2: Ali’s Halves on May 4 ............................................................................... 67 Figure 3: Ionah's Way to Show One-half on May 4 ................................................ 82 Figure 4: Hannah’s Representation for a Fraction on May 3 ................................. 83 Figure 5: Rob's Geoboard Of 1 / 2 and 2/ 4 on May 7 ............................................... 85 Figure 6: Halves Referred to by Shawna on May 7 ................................................ 87 Figure 7: Moira Continues to Outline ”Squares” on May 7 ................................... 88 Figure 8: A Way to Use Partial Squares to Prove Half. .......................................... 88 Figure 9: Overhead Geoboard Referred to by Rob to Indicate Fourths, May 7 91 Figure 10: Monte's Drawing Of Groups on May 3. ................................................. 95 Figure 11: Kim's Diamond on May 11 ..................................................................... 97 Figure 12: Jody’s Attempt to Combine Triangles to Make a Square on May12. 101 Figure 13: Hannah's Drawing of 1 / 2 on May 4 .................................................... 108 Figure 14: Hannah's TV (Drawn from Side View) on May 10 ............................. 111 Figure 15: Monica's Conjecture on May 10 ........................................................... 117 Figure 16: Monica’s Drawing of 3/5 on May 10 ................................................... 118 Figure 17: Monte’s Drawing of Groups on May 3 ................................................ 127 Figure 18: Hannah's Representation for a Fraction on May 3 ............................. 129 Figure 19: Michaela's Way to Make Half on May 7 ............................................... 155 Figure 20: Antonia's and Braden's Ways to Make Half on May 7 ....................... 158 vii CHAPTER ONE INTRODUCTION TO THE STUDY Recent reform documents call for changes in the teaching and learning Of mathematics (National Council Of Teachers of Mathematics [NCTM], 1989, 1991; National Research Council [NRC], 1989). Written from a constructivist perspective, these documents assert that what students learn and how they learn it is personal and influenced by the learning environment provided by teachers. Different from the traditional view Of teaching as telling, these reforms seek to increase student involvement in mathematical activity and discourse. Reformers believe that this heightened involvement will enhance students' mathematical power, defined as, The ability to explore, conjecture, and reason logically; to solve non- routine problems' to communicate about and through mathematics; and to connect ideas within mathematics and between mathematics and other intellectual activity. Mathematical power also involves the development Of personal self-confidence and a disposition tO seek, evaluate, and use quantitative and spatial information in solving problems and in making decisions. Students' flexibility, perseverance, interest, curiosity, and inventiveness also affect the realization Of mathematical power (NC'I'M, 1991, p. 1). These documents advocate increased student understandings Of mathematics and view discourse - ”ways Of representing, thinking, talking, and agreeing and disagreeing” - as a powerful process through which to develop and enhance students' mathematical power. In my study, mathematical discourse reveals itself primarily through whole-group discussion. In discussion, these students are provided the tools (physical as well as intellectual) with which to represent their ideas. However, what it actually means for teachers to promote discourse with the goal Of increased student mathematical power, is an area Of study in its infancy. Whereas all classrooms have discourse, the discourse described in reform documents requires different roles and responsibilities Of teachers and students. NO longer are teachers to tell while students passively receive knowledge. The advocated discourse increases students' role in the construction Of their understandings and draws upon disputes over ideas as ways to refine and develop ideas. Controversies Of the kind that the Standards (1991) seem to promote are not common in most classrooms; to the contrary, student consensus and acceptance are more the rule. My study focuses on an element Of mathematical discourse, disagreement. As cited above, discourse encompasses ways Of representing, thinking, talking, and agreeing and disagreeing. While each construct (i.e., representing, thinking, talking, agreeing, disagreeing) brings to mind particular classroom images, it is the notion Of disagreeing on which I want to focus. Why is the study Of disagreeing, or disagreement important to pursue? First, there appears to be little research emphasis on disagreement in general, what it is or what goes on during mathematical disagreement. Whereas disagreement may be simply defined as those moments when two or more people do not agree or have the same idea, disagreement may also be thought Of as dynamic and complex. Second, little is understood about the ways in which students’ disagreements arise and are managed. Rather than encouraging lesson-centered disagreement, many teachers may wish to avoid disagreements as deterrents to the forward motion of a predetermined lesson plan. In addition, those moments when disagreements arise may be unnerving to teachers as times when they are unsure about whether to manage the mathematics, the social nature Of who is disagreeing and in what ways, or both. For example, are some disagreements centered around mathematics and others more personal? In order for disagreements among students to take place, teachers must relinquish some Of the previously-held control over discourse and lesson direction, to allow students' ideas and questions to have some influence over where the mathematics leads and to provide students the discourse space within which to explore and own their thinking and mathematical understandings. What is learned and how it is learned may become more student-centered when disagreements are permitted and nurtured. Third, when examined, disagreements may be revealed as a rich source for understanding the complexities Of mathematics teaching and learning. AS alluded in the Standards (1991), disagreement embedded within discourse may be a window for teachers into students’ mathematical learning, at the same time force children to articulate and justify their reasoning for themselves and for others. A give and take may result as different ideas make their way to the floor. In the process, children may also learn respect for each others’ thinking. ”It seems possible that in our attempts to say things clearly to other people we progressively learn to build their viewpoints into our own, and thus to see our knowledge (or perceptions) as hypothetical and Open to change” (Barnes, 1992). In other words, if an audience Of peers holds children accountable for their thinking, explanation and articulation spring tO the forefront as important benefits of disagreement. Finally, the very existence Of student disagreement in an elementary mathematics classroom is interesting and important to pursue because it takes place in a subject commonly perceived tO be consensual. Our culture carries the belief that in school mathematics, at least, there is one known, correct answer to each problem posed. Therefore, the notion that disagreement is planned for is counterintuitive-some would say counterproductive—in a consensual discipline such as mathematics. In addition, these disagreements exist in a discourse space created by one fourth-grade teacher, Katherine Vandenberg, a space within which students are encouraged to share ideas, clarify, justify, explain, defend, and disagree, through speech, journal writing, and use Of manipulatives, whether in whole-group discussion, small-group discussion, individual contemplation, or one-tO-0ne interaction with her or with one another. This study is about student-to-student disagreement in a fourth-grade class. This study Of disagreement may also be thought Of as one of argument, in the broadest sense. For within argument, which ”refers broadly and neutrally to reasoning in order to make decisions or test the truth Of claims” (Perkins, p. 155), is subsumed such things as explore, clarify, justify, explain, defend, and disagree-characteristics of mathematical power advocated by reform documents. My exploratory study of student disagreement begins to examine closely the issues Cited above. Little is known about a) disagreement in general; b) how individual disagreements during discourse arise and how they are managed; c) the potential richness Of teaching and learning that exists during student disagreement; d) the roles Of teachers and students during disagreements, and; e) the existence of disagreement in a subject commonly perceived to have known, agreed-upon answers. This study discusses how during disagreements students are forced to clarify and defend the reasoning behind their solutions, and through this process they gain at least some of the increased mathematical power which reform documents call for. When student disagreement arises during whole-group discussion, it is imperative that educators (and reformers) have some sense Of this complex discourse process in which students are expected to gain mathematical power, and to recognize what students need to learn in order to be involved disagreement. This study looked closely at a Specific component Of discourse, student disagreement, for the purpose Of understanding what it takes to be involved in it in this classroom. CHAPTER TWO LITERATURE REVIEW As I wrote in Chapter One, little is known about disagreement during whole-group discussion — about disagreement in general, how disagreements arise and are managed, the potential richness Of teaching and learning that exists during student disagreement, the roles Of teachers and students during disagreements, and the existence Of disagreement in a subject commonly perceived to have known, agreed-upon answers. In this chapter I outline my rationale for the sociolinguistic study Of disagreement in mathematics discussions. I follow with a review Of relevant literature. In the first part Of the chapter I discuss my theoretical framework Of sociocultural learning theory as the basis of my study. Next I discuss my study’s analytical framework which is grounded in sociolinguistics. In the following three sections Of the chapter I discuss classroom discourse, mathematical discourse, and mathematics (respectively) and highlight the need for research in these areas. These sections follow the others to underscore the need for studies Of mathematical discourse and specifically, student disagreement. I close the chapter with a discussion of the literature on argument and disagreement. Theoretical Framework To examine student disagreement, this study is grounded in sociocultural learning theory, particularly the work Of Vygotsky (1962, 1978) which emphasizes the importance Of language in learning. In this study Of student disagreement, children’s use Of language to negotiate meaning is central. For Vygotsky (1978), language was the most important psychological tool or sign. As a sign, language mediates learning. That is, it is capable Of transforming mental functioning. According to this theory, there are three basic themes regarding learning and language (W ertsch, 1985). The first theme is based on the premise that in order to understand development Of children researchers must focus on the process Of development. That is, ”the study Of behavior is not an auxiliary aspect of theoretical study, but rather forms its very base" (Vygotsky, 1978, p. 65). In this view, behavior is a process and not a product, and appropriate method takes this into consideration. The second theme Of Vygotsky’s learning theory is the premise that learning first occurs in the social domain. In other words, meaning originates between individuals. ”Every function in the child’s cultural development appears twice: first, on the social level, and later, on the individual level: first between people (inter-psychological), and then inside the child (intrapsychological)” (1978, p. 57). That is, through speech in the social or external domain, the child transforms and internalizes what is learned. What the child knew previously is internally reconstructed. In Vygotsky’s view (1978), internalization is not a unidirectional transfer process. TO the contrary, he viewed it as a series of transformations: (a) An Operation that initially represents an external activity is reconstructed and begins to occur internally. (b) An interpersonal process is transformed into an intrapersonal one. (c) The transformation of an interpersonal process into an intrapersonal one is the result Of a long series Of developmental events (p. 56-57). This series of transformations moves back and forth, from the external to the internal, from the interpersonal to the intrapersonal. Gradually, as transformation continues, internalization is the result. Inherent in sociocultural learning theory is the premise that individuals use talk and action (i.e., behavior) in focal and dynamic ways. In the case Of mathematical discourse, the implication is that discussion contributes to student learning, a notion highlighted in recent reform documents (National Council Of Teachers Of Mathematics [NCTM], 1989, 1991 ; National Research Council [NRC], 1989). The third theme Of Vygotsky’s learning theory is the claim that to understand thinking (”mental processes”), we must understand the language used to mediate it. In his view, language (”signs”) mediates mental processes. In other words, through language use learning takes place. In addition to the above themes, Vygotsky (1978) claimed that learning was guided by a more knowledgeable other through a zone Of proximal (i.e., potential) development. He wrote that the zone of proximal development ”is the distance between the actual developmental level as determined by independent problem solving and the level Of potential development as determined through problem solving under adult guidance or in collaboration with more capable peers” (p. 86). In practical terms, the zone Of proximal development is the distance between what a child knows about, for example, fractions, and what a child can learn through discourse about fractions. In the instance Of whole-group discussion, a teacher creates different zones Of proximal development which lead to mathematical understandings. That is, ”learning awakens a variety Of internal developmental processes that are able tO Operate only when the child is interacting with people in his environment and in cooperation with his peers. Once these processes are internalized, they become part Of the child’s independent developmental achievement” (p. 90). From this perspective, experiences on the social level activate internal processes. Together, these Vygotskian themes underng this study of fourth- graders’ disagreement, because to understand what disagreement is and how children are involved warrants close examination Of language use and specifically, disagreement. When public disagreement occurs during whole- group discussion, the discussion as orchestrated by the teacher creates different zones of proximal development through which children’s learning occurs. In addition, when children disagree, they push the outer limits of these zones, forcing one another to explain and clarify what is said and why. A sociocultural study of language use includes a study of the dynamic use Of speech as communication. And Vygotsky was concerned that the interrelationship between communicative and intellectual functions Of speech be recognized: The initial function of speech is the communicative function. Speech is first and foremost a means Of social interaction, a means of pronouncement and understanding. This function Of speech, which is usually analyzed in terms of isolated units, has been separated from the intellectual function Of speech. Both functions are ascribed to speech as if they were parallel to or independent Of one another. Speech, as it were, combines within itself both the function Of social interaction and the function of thinking, but what the relationship is between these two functions, what brings about the presence of the two functions of speech, how they develop, and how they are structurally intertwined are questions that have remained uninvestigated (Vygotsky, 1934, p. 88, cited in Wertsch, 1985, p. 94). Vygotsky as cited in Wertsch (1985) wrote that the two functions of speech - communicative (social) and intellectual (thinking) — are commonly treated as separate (parallel or independent) entities. In addition, he said this shortchanges the dynamic character Of speech. In his view, the focus Of speech studies should be the relationship between the two functions Of speech. Let me provide an example to illustrate Vygotsky’s statements about the inherent relationship between the two functions Of speech (communicative and intellectual). A parallel example Of research on disagreement in elementary mathematics would be an investigation of the relationship between disagreement (e.g., what it is, how it arises, how it is managed) and its function in the discussion (e. g., to converge mathematical thinking, to diverge mathematical thinking). What is the appropriate method to conduct studies of the relationship between the intellectual and social functions of speech (or Of the relationship between disagreement and its function in the discussion)? In the next section I discuss the analytical framework Of this study. In addition I continue to build the rationale for my study Of student disagreement. Analytical Framework Separately and in combination, I used sociocultural learning theory and the method of sociolinguistics - both Of which emphasize the central role Of language in making meaning - tO shed light on disagreement in the setting Of a fourth-grade mathematics class. For my analytical framework I drew on the method Of sociolinguistics, particularly the techniques Of discourse analysis. Gumperz (1994) wrote that sociolinguistics is a method to investigate the language use of particular human groups (p. 9). Similarly, Erickson (1986) defined the method as grounded in the language use Of participants, and the ”immediate and local meaning Of actions, as defined from the actor’s point Of 10 view” (p. 119). As a method, sociolinguistics focuses on the relationship between speakers and hearers (as well as between writers and readers). Within the analytical framework Of sociolinguistics, discourse analysis is a cluster Of techniques to analyze language use. These techniques are used to investigate language use from the participants’ perspective. For example, researchers Of classroom discourse strive to understand the perspective Of teachers and students while they draw on the techniques Of discourse analysis to investigate its various aspects, such as: speech structure, speech event, speech acts, intonation, conversational analysis, topic, questions, discourse markers, or participants’ use of pronouns. The goal is to use an interpretive lens to look for patterns and relationships which emerge from the data to account for participants’ perspective. About research on language use in classrooms, Gee, Michaels, and O’ Connor (1992) wrote that it is logical to begin with discourse in order to use it as evidence Of a social and academic processes. Mehan (1979) would term these processes as competencies — academic and social. A potential social and academic process to investigate is disagreement in mathematics discussions. TO participate in discussion and disagreement, children need to learn how to be involved in it. Below I discuss participant structure, participation norms, turn-taking patterns, and floor - all Of which must be fostered by the teacher in order for the children in this study to be able to disagree. Participant Structure TO be involved in mathematical disagreement during discussions, children must learn what to say and when. That is, they must appropriate the 11 participant structures Of the setting. Defined by Erickson and Shultz (1981) as ”differing configurations of concerted action,” participant structures inherently include norms Of participation. These norms are continually negotiated by participants. In short, participation norms are ”the rights and Obligations Of participants with respect to who can say what, when, and tO whom" (Cazden, 1986, p. 437). In the fourth grade setting of mathematics discussions, the rights and Obligations Of the children included knowing if they could speak about their fractions ideas, when it was appropriate, and who they should address. TO disagree, this knowledge is essential while children negotiated meaning-in- action (Erickson, 1986). And to negotiate meaning-in-action, children needed to appropriate participation norms, or patterns Of teacher-student interaction. Teachers do not necessarily Openly teach participation norms, nor teach all Of them. Some may be indexed (Mehan, 1979), that is, learned about implicitly through teacher statements, such as, ”When someone is up here sharing their ideas and we’re calling out or talking, that shows them we’re not interested in their idea. That’ s not acceptable. Right now you’re supposed to be listening” (Katherine Vandenberg, April 28). As stated in the previous chapter, discourse Of the type advocated in reform documents requires different roles and responsibilities of teachers and students. With increased involvement in discourse as students directly address one another, classroom discourse becomes conversation-like. In part it becomes conversation-like because student-initiated turn taking becomes a participation norm. According tO Sacks, Schegloff, and Jefferson (1978), turns may be initiated in any Of three ways. First, an individual may self-select a turn to talk. By speaking up during or near a juncture or pause in the talk, the person who 12 wishes to speak gains the floor. Second, the next speaker may be chosen by the current speaker. For example, when I direct a question to a student, I might conclude my turn with the question, 'What do you think, Amanda?" thereby indicating specifically who I expect to speak next. Finally, the current speaker may continue when the other speaker does not initiate a turn during or following a pause after the speaking turn. Violation Of these three turn-initiation procedures may result in simultaneous speech in the form Of interruption or back-channeling (Murray, 1985).1 Once a speaking turn is initiated, according to Sacks, Schegloff, and Jefferson (1978), the speaker is said to have speaker's rights until he or she concludes the speaking turn. Whether for a one-syllable utterance (e.g., "Oh?", "I-Imm") on the one hand, or a lengthy diatribe on the other, once initiated, a speaker has the conversational right to complete his or her turn. When a speaker's rights are violated some form Of repair is necessary for the conversation to proceed. A common repair mechanism during simultaneous speech is when one speaker stops talking so that the other may continue. When a speaker has the floor, a turn may not go on endlessly. Completion rights hinge on "how long 3 / he has been speaking, how Often s/ he has spoken, the number Of 'points' made in a speaking turn, and the special rights of some speakers to speak about some topics" (Murray, 1985, p. 31). In summary, conversations are means by which individuals—adults and children - communicate. Sacks, Schegloff, and Jefferson (1978) asserted that lWhereas interruption as simultaneous speech is typically interpreted as a violation of a speaker's rights (Coulthard, 1988; West 6: Zimmerman, 1983; West & Garcia, 1988), back-Channeling is usually considered a hearer's cooperative, supportive comments to the current speaker. 13 conversations are context-free speech events which involve interaction between at least two people who typically acknowledge and honor rights to speak during turns at talk. However, their assertion that conversations are context free has been challenged (see for example, West & Garcia, 1988; West 8: Zimmerman, 1983). In addition, their assertion that turn taking is orderly, discussed below, has been challenged (Edelsky, 1993). Teachers and students in classrooms also follow rules for structuring discourse. In what ways is classroom discourse similar to or different from conversations in other settings? We know that school lessons involve discourse, and that classroom discourse, similar to conversation, involves the interaction between at least two people (e. g., teacher and student, student and student). What other characteristics are shared by conversation and classroom discourse? Classroom Discourse Cazden (1986) described spoken language as "the medium by which much teaching takes place and in which students demonstrate to teachers much of what they know" (p. 432). As situated language used in a particular setting, classroom discourse during teacher-led lessons in this country is most characterized by the basic interactional pattern between teachers and students, the Initiation-Response—Feedback (IRF) sequencez. Within the three-part IRF sequence teachers initiate (I) questions, receive responses from students (R), and follow up those responses (F). Directed by the teacher, the IRF pattern is the most distinguishable feature that separates classroom discourse from conversation. 2Pimm (1987) also refers to this pattern as IRF; however, Cazden(1988) and Mehan (1979) refer to this pattern as IRE (teacher initiation-student response-teacher evaluation). Whether termed IRF or IRE, these authors refer to the same three-part interactional pattern between teachers and students. 14 "All analyses of teacher-led classroom discourse find examples of this pattern, and anyone hearing it recognizes it as classroom talk and not just informal conversation" (Cazden, 1988, p. 30). Whereas the IRF sequence may be used by teachers to control classroom talk, it may be used alternatively in discussions (Wells, 1993). This three-part sequence is not necessarily a three-turn sequence. In other words, when the goal is increased involvement of children in discussion (as opposed to limited involvement) a teacher may initiate the sequence with a question and seek multiple responses through further questions. Or, she may seek multiple responses by connecting the responses of several children. During the feedback or follow-up part of the sequence, a teacher may use it to take ”an opportunity to extend the student’s answer, to draw out its significance, or to make connections with other parts of the students’ total experience during the unit” (p. 30). Possibly, a teacher would use the follow-up for revoicing (Michaels 8: O’ Connor, 1993). During improvisational discourse, revoicing as a discourse move used by teachers functions to (a) provide the teacher an opportunity to draw inference from a student contribution, (b) provide the responding student the right to validate that inference, and (c) position the responding student and his or her contribution in relationship to others’ contributions. When the IRF sequence breaks out of the three-turn sequencing pattern, classroom discourse takes on new potential. Classroom discourse then has the potential to become more give-and-take and conversation-like. It is not the same as conversation, however, this breakout pattern allows for self-selection by students of turns to get ideas on the floor. 15 In the end, classroom talk is focused on subject matter, for the (Often debated) goal of schooling in the United States is to educate children in the. core subject areas of mathematics, science, language arts, and social sciences. Since the focus of school lessons is teaching and learning of subject matter, it is crucial that students and teachers work toward bringing subject-specific knowledge into the public arena. However, in order to demonstrate their subject-matter knowledge during IRF sequences or at any other time during school lessons, students must also learn what to say, when and how to say it, and to whom. In other words, an overall classroom competence is required. [C]lassroom competence is not limited to academic matters. . .classroom competence involves matters of form as well as content. TO be successful in the classroom, students not only must know the content of academic subjects, they must learn the appropriate form in which to cast their academic knowledge. That is, competent membership in the classroom community involves employing interactional skills and abilities in the display of academic knowledge. They must know with whom, when, and where they can speak and act, and they must provide the speech and behavior that are appropriate for a given classroom situation (Mehan, 1979, p. 133). Mehan's (1979) notion of classroom competence consists of two critical elements: academic competence and social competence. Academic or subject- matter learning is the basic function of schooling and is what distinguishes education from other settings; knowledge of social skills and language use is required for students to succeed as competent members Of the classroom. Competence in one area without accompanying competence in the other results in students' failure to gain the floor for the purpose Of making contributions to the flow of the lesson. That is, in order for students to get their academic knowledge on the floor, they must have the social know-how to gain the floor. Having gained the floor, their voices can be heard and knowledge made visible l6 and public. Successful participation in lessons then appears to include both aspects Of classroom competence: public display of subject-matter knowledge and understanding Of the social means by which one's voice may be heard. The above discussion of academic and social competence resonates with the basis of sociocultural learning theory - the use of language to negotiate meaning (Vygotsky, 1978). The two competencies also resonate with the earlier quotation from Wertsch (1985). In that quotation he called for studies of the relationship between the communicative and intellectual functions of speech. In addition, the competencies are subsumed by sociolinguistics as a method to investigate language use of participants in discourse (Erickson, 1986; Gumperz, 1994). As stated above, in order to share one's academic knowledge, students must first gain the floor. "Having a turn to talk is the minimal requisite for influencing the course of a lesson. This involves getting the floor. However, students cannot just talk any time" (Mehan, 1979, p. 140). A significant part of students' classroom competence is knowing when an appropriate juncture, or "a break (silence) between turns of talk" (Mehan, p. 140) emerges. It is at these junctures that students tend to attempt to gain the floor. At this point it is appropriate to ask how students gain the floor in order to make their contributions to the classroom talk. Mehan (1979) included the following as means by which students may gain the floor for a turn at talk: 1. Wait for a direct nomination to speak from the teacher. In this turn- allocation procedure, the teacher calls on specific students to speak. The student called upon therefore has the right to speak and complete his or her turn. For example, consider the following: 17 KV: A half split? Austin, can you add to that some? Did you think it was both? Austin: Yea. (Transcript, May 3). During her speaking turn the teacher restated and attributed an idea suggested in the previous turn by a student, and nominated Austin to provide further comment. Cooperating with this school-related format, Austin waited until he was called on to speak. 2. Resmnd to the teacher's invitation to bid. Frequently during lessons teachers invite students to bid through the use of particular phrases or questions. Whereas bidding typically takes the form of hand raising (which is more acceptable), it occasionally includes student callouts (which tend to be perceived as disruptive and less acceptable). For example, students may make a statement that is followed by the teacher's question, "Nobody has an idea about 200 + 10?” (Transcript, May 3) or "What do you guys think?" (Transcript, May 3). In the act of asking these general questions wherein no particular student is addressed the teacher opens the invitation for students to bid for the floor. Students may also initiate speaking turns by bidding directly for the floor. The disruptive student callouts mentioned above belong in this category. At times these call outs are picked up (i.e., uptake, or an immediate and direct response [Coulthard, 1988]) by the teacher or other students and the discussion will continue. Occasionally such call outs are unacknowledged. However, there are times when a speaking turn is underway and invitations to bid for the floor are not Opened up by teachers. During these times, students may spontaneously raise their hands (sometimes accompanied by vocal "Oh, Oh!" or "Ooh, ooh!") and 18 begin to call out a comment while the teacher or a fellow classmate is speaking, thereby indicating that they have something to add to what has been said and are willing to talk. However, in these student-initiated bids for the floor, basic turn-taking rules are generally followed. That is, students tend to allow their fellow classmates the opportunity and the right to complete their turn. Students may border on interruption when they noisily raise their hands, lean forward, and start to call out in hopes Of being called on. When they do not honor speakers' rights they risk public recognition that their actions are disruptive to the flow of the lesson, as in the following exchange: KV: That’ s not appropriate to call out. Shhh. Boys and girls stop and listen. When someone’s up here [at the overhead] sharing their ideas and we’re calling out or talking to our table mates that shows them that we’re not interested in their ideas. That’s not acceptable. Right now you’re supposed to be taking notes on these ideas that we’re generating and listening. I see almost half Of Table 2 hasn't written anything yet (Transcript, April 28). As she spoke, Mrs. Vandenberg appeared to honor students’ desire to speak at the same time She stated her interest in the attempted contribution of the child at the overhead. However, attempts in this situation to gain the floor were determined to be impolite at a time when Mrs. Vandenberg considered it critical to listen to others for the purpose of the ensuing discussion. To summarize this section on classroom discourse, several characteristics of classroom communication emerge. First, discourse in American classrooms is typically led by the teacher who controls speaking turns for the most part. Second, a common feature of classroom discourse is the IRF, or teacher initiation- student response-teacher feedback, pattern of interaction. During these sequences, teachers ask a question, seek a response from a student, and follow 19 the student's response with a brief (sometimes evaluative) comment. Third, in order to share the requisite subject-matter knowledge during lessons, students are required to be have two types of competence: academic and social. Knowledge of what to say fails without requisite knowledge of who to say it to, when, and in what way. And, students are required to know when and how to get ideas on the floor. What is ”floor,” and how do children get ideas on it? Typically referred to and often discussed in studies of classroom discourse (see for example, Cazden, 1988; Mehan, 1979; Shultz, Florio, 8: Erickson, 1982), the concept of floor has recently changed. Until recently, floor was considered to be ill-defined, at times ”defined as a speaker, a turn, and control over part of a conversation” (Edelsky, 1993, p. 205). This definition was confusing and not helpful in explaining the meaning participants make of talk. Taking meaning-in-action of participants into consideration, Edelsky defined floor as ”the acknowledged what’s-going-on within a psychological time/space” (p. 209). Edelsky argued that floor is mutually negotiated by participants in talk, and is not the discrete, orderly phenomenon ‘ discussed by others (Sacks, Schegloff, 8: Jefferson, 1978). Her collaborative definition of floor differs from previous competitive models and captures the dynamic and interactive nature of floor. Edelsky (1993) also differentiated between floor and turn, previously and loosely defined as one-at-a-time, orderly talk. By contrast, Edelsky wrote that turn is not necessarily orderly. Nor is it necessarily discrete because frequently in conversation speakers overlap one another so that the end Of a turn is determined to be when a researcher decided, not necessarily when the speaker did. Edelsky defined turn as ”on-record ’speaking’ (which may include nonverbal 20 activities) behind which lies an intention to convey a message that is both referential and functional” (p. 207). Edelsky’s definition of floor and turn allow for the simultaneous examination of turns and ideas on the floor. In other words, researchers may examine those instances where turns may come and go, but ideas remain on the floor through several turns. When classroom discourse becomes give-and-take and conversation-like and teachers foster norms Of participation which allow for children to talk directly to one another, it is reasonable to assume that some of students’ talk will be exploratory (Barnes, 1992). Exploratory speech is what the term implies, speech whereby children test ideas and assumptions. In essence, children may use exploratory speech to rehearse aloud their thinking, ”to rearrange their thoughts during improvised talk” (p. 108). What does it mean for classroom discourse to be student-centered when it is grounded in subject matter? What challenges do teachers of mathematics face when they have as their goal increased student involvement in discourse while also fostering the norms Of participation discussed above? In the next section I discuss recent research on teaching in the spirit of reform. Mathematical Discourse As discussed in Chapter One, reform documents favor discourse with increased student involvement. ”Not commonly viewed as a discursive subject” (Pimm, 1987, p. 47), mathematics advocated by authors of reform stands in contrast to traditional school mathematics. Research on the complexities of teaching in the spirit of reform sheds light on different roles and responsibilities of teachers and students. Reform-oriented research details some of the 21 challenges faced by teachers and students when they experience the kind of discourse envisioned by reformers. Research on teaching in the spirit of the Stanggrgg (NCTM, 1989, 1991) has highlighted the challenges in doing so (Ball, 1990, 1991, 1993; Ball 8: Wilson, 1996; Lampert, 1986, 1987; Lampert, Rittenhouse, 8: Crumbaugh, 1996). Drawing on their practice to engage in research on teaching and learning, the authors simultaneously focused on teaching, subject matter, learning, and community. In Ball’s words ”My ears and eyes must search the world around us, the discipline of mathematics, and the world of the child with both mathematical and child filters” (Ball, 1990, p. 27). In addition, practical and moral dilemmas abound in this kind of teaching. For example, posing the question, What’s all this talk about discourse?, Ball (1991) underscored the point that the Standards (NCTM, 1989, 1991) cannot be prescriptive. To the contrary, she wrote that the Standards are a guide, a set of tools for use by teachers to construct a conception of good teaching. In addition, the Standards are a springboard for productive conversations about teaching. To Ball, ”the discourse of a classroom is formed by students and the teacher and the tools with which they work” (p. 44). In this sense, discourse is situation specific, hinging on the actions of participants. This formulation is highlighted by the individuality by which discourse is orchestrated. In other words, there are no rules that teachers may follow in order to conduct mathematical discussions. Each teacher must construct an understanding of the subject matter Of mathematics - and an understanding of the M. And, practice is laden with decisions - often dilemmas -- about what to do and when. For example, teachers are constantly considering what questions to ask and how to ask them, 22 what mathematical aspect of students’ contributions to focus on, what an appropriate representation is, and what knowledge they have of students and the diversity they represent (Ball 8: Wilson, 1996). And, for teachers, this means thinking about teaching and learning as relational. Although Ball (1991) does not explicitly address the notion of relationships among learners, the concept of relational learning is implied in the following: The classroom environment, or culture, that the students and teacher construct affects the discourse in some important ways. The environment shapes how safe students feel, whether and how they respect one another and themselves, and the extent to which serious engagement in mathematical thinking is the norm (p. 45). New kinds of discursive relationships among students must be fostered so that Children learn how to speak to one another about their mathematical ideas. The goal is for heightened involvement in mathematical discourse to enhance students' mathematical power (as proposed by the Standards [NCTM, 1989, 1991]). Burbules (1993) used the term ”dialogical relation” to describe a relation between people in the context of discussion, a relation ”to ’carry away’ its participants, to ’catch them up’ in an interaction that takes on a force and direction of its own, often leading them beyond any intended goal to new and unexpected insights” (p. 20). Lampert and colleagues (1986, 1989, 1992; Lampert et. a1, 1996) also draw on the teacher's perspective to research mathematical teaching and learning. Similar to Ball (1990, 1991, 1993) Lampert’s research on mathematics teaching and learning raises questions about the changing roles of teachers and students in mathematical discourse of the kind advocated in reform documents. Lampert’s research also captures the complex demands placed on teachers to 23 draw on mathematical knowledge and pedagogy in conjunction with the accompanying need to constantly listen to and make sense of students’ developing mathematical ideas. And, these authors wrote, heightened involvement Of children in mathematical discourse sometimes brought disagreement. When children disagreed, additional uncertainties emerged which could affect a child’s feeling of competence (Ball 8: Wilson, 1996) or discomfort (Lampert, Rittenhouse, 8: Crumbaugh, 1996). In essence, the research by Ball (1990, 1991, 1993, 1996) and Lampert (1986, 1989, 1992, 1996) captures a cognitive perception or disposition (Bereiter, 1997) toward teaching and learning which is at the core of teaching in the spirit of the m (1989, 1991). As Lampert (1986) wrote, ”What seems most important is that teachers and students together are disposed toward a particular way of viewing and doing mathematics in the classroom” (p. 329). In a sense these researchers draw attention to the fact that in this kind of teaching there is more to mathematical discourse than mathematics. While ”managing the intermental” (O’Connor, 1996), or interpsychological domain of learning, these researchers point to areas of focus for teachers who wish to teach in the spirit of reform. Regarding the role of the teacher, it would seem that there is more required than subject matter knowledge. Knowledge of children and how to manage discussions also figure into the teaching equation. 24 Curriculum Teachers who wish to align their practice with current reform need curriculum resources. One resource for teaching fractions is fling Frgg'ogs and was developed and piloted in California (Corwin, Russell, 8: Tierney, 1990). The authors developed this resource around critical ideas in fractions: ‘A fraction can be used to describe all kinds of situations, including parts of one thing, parts of a group of things, and rates. ‘Different fractions can express the same relationships. *Estimation of the size of a fraction or the results Of an Operation with fractions is vital to understanding fractions. *In part-whole situations, equal fractions Of a whole are the same size; when dividing something into halves or into fourths, the unit is divided into equal pieces. However, those pieces need not look the same. ”The comparative order of fractions is absolute, but in part-whole situations their size is relative to the size of their units. 'To understand a part-whole model, you have to keep in mind the whole, even when the whole disappears and only the fraction is being used. *In part-whole situations, fractions which are larger than unit fractions (fractions with 1 in the numerator) are constructed by repeating the unit fraction (e.g., 2/ 3 is 1/ 3 and 1/3). *When fractions are used to express rates, the same rate describes a series of ratios. (p. 24) Citing the Curriculum and Evaluation Standards (NCTM, 1989), Corwin et. al (1990) wrote that NCTM saw "five major goals for students as: "learning to value mathematics 'becoming confident in one’s own ability *becoming a mathematical problem solver *learning to communicate mathematically *learning to reason mathematically (p. 5). The authors went on to discuss teaching for understanding. In their view children should be able to construct mathematical models, teachers should not tell, children (like mathematicians) should be able to talk and write about their ideas, and frustration from children is to be expected (Corwin et. al, 1990, p. 6-7). 25 About the connection between the frustration Of mathematicians and the frustration of students, they wrote, Approaches [of adult mathematicians] to mathematical problems are tried, explored, rejected, talked about, compared, and changed. The same should be true of our students - they need to try things, to come up against walls, and to try in a different way. . . It is hard to allow our students to struggle with their frustration. . . Helping your students make sense of their own methods, helping them express their confusion, and helping them learn from each other’s approaches will give them much more than telling them will do (p. 8). Although Corwin et. al (1990) point to student frustration, they do not provide teachers insight into what to do when it arises. Nor do they detail what to do when children disagree. Corwin et. al provide teachers with the rationale for teaching fractions through activities which are aligned with the vision of reform. This rationale is discussed up front, apparently to lay the foundation for the open-ended tasks that follow. The rationale and tasks in in Fra ' are different from many curriculum resources in elementary mathematics. There is no script Of what the teacher should say, followed by what students can be expected to say. Nor is there a list of problems given at the end of a module (e.g., geometry, rates, mixed numbers). By contrast, Corwin et. al describe and discuss activities with an eye on the mathematics (i.e., fractions content) in the activities. Timely questions about mathematical discourse linger: What is disagreement? What do teachers do when it arises? How do teachers manage disagreement? I next discuss studies of argument and studies of disagreement in mathematics classrooms. 26 Argument and Disagreement As I wrote in the last chapter, this study of disagreement may also be thought of in the broadest sense as one of argument. For within argument, which "refers broadly and neutrally to reasoning in order to make decisions or test the truth of claims" (Perkins, p. 155), is subsumed such things as explore, clarify, justify, explain, defend, and disagree - characteristics of mathematical power advocated by the authors Of reform documents. In the adult world, an argument is a chain of reasoning on which decisions are made or the truth of claims are tested (Perkins, 1986). Toulmin (1958) wrote that argument is logic and has an identifiable structure: Argument is like an organism. It has both a gross, anatomical structure and a finer, as-it-were physiological one. When set out explicitly in all its detail, it may occupy a number of printed pages or take perhaps a quarter of an hour to deliver; and within this time or space one can distinguish the main phases marking the progress of the argument from the initial statement of an unsettled problem to the final presentation of a conclusion (p. 94). In his book, The Uses of Argument, Toulmin (1958) primarily discussed argument as a final product (as Opposed to a process). For example, he detailed the many components of an argument and their inter-relatedness. These components of argument include assertion, claim, warrants, backing, data, qualifier, and so on. Purportedly, he detailed the components of argument to provide a context in which scholars could concentrate on the finer points of argument. This concentration, he believed, would nudge logicians toward a tighter fit between the finer points of argument and their ”gross, anatomical structure.” Although readers may infer that it is people who make arguments, 27 person-to-person interaction over the development of arguments is essentially absent from Toulmin’s description. Perkins (1986) described argument as design — ”a structure shaped to a purpose” (p. 156). This structure can be as simple as the interaction between children, as in the following: Why? Because. Perkins (1986) went on to say that the purpose of argument is to specify ”why?" Perkins also implied a critical aspect of argument in connection with this study: the messiness Of argument-in-action. To compare the formal logic of mathematics with informal arguments (i.e., akin to disagreement between children) Perkins wrote: In the formal logic Of mathematics. . . the truth Of the premises is a matter Of assumption, but in informal argument about real-world matters, the truth of premises is bound to be in question from time to time. . . [I]nformal arguments are messy and uncertain in ways that formal arguments are not (p. 163-164). Above, Perkins (1986) highlighted the exploratory direction in which disagreement may take children. And, he positioned the idea of argument-in- action in the real world, a world which includes children using language to explore mathematical ideas. What does it mean for children to argue or disagree (the child version of argue) in mathematics discussions? Wilkinson and Martino (1993) examined students’ disagreements in one small group during mathematics. In their work they based their definition of disagreement episodes on the work of Lindow, Wilkinson, and Peterson (1984). In the view of Wilkinson and Martino, disagreement episodes were ”the interaction that followed a verbal assertion of 28 not agreeing with an answer or a step in arriving at an answer to a mathematics problem” (p. 144). Using a coding system, the authors reviewed transcripts and video tapes to identify and isolate disagreements. In addition, they defined disagreement as a three-part process: (1) antecedent position, (2) disagreement, and (3) resolution moves. Resolution moves included consensus and lack of consensus. Consensus was indicated by verbal acceptance of a previous statement. Lack Of consensus was indicated either verbally or nonverbally. Disagreement was coded as ended ”when consensus was reached or when the interaction shifted to a new topic” (p. 145). The coded data was triangulated with additional sources, including results from the mathematics portion of a standardized test and the results of a sociometric interview. Results showed that two of the four children in the study acted as catalysts in disagreement. That is, they ”freely initiated and participated in disagreements as well as group discussions” (p. 159). Both students also received high sociometric scores from their classmates. They were perceived as leaders, and their answers and solutions were attended to by other members of the small group. In addition, Wilkinson and Martino (1993) found that one particular problem (i.e., the cube problem) produced more and better mathematical communication. The cube problem was designed by a research team led by Karen Fuson (1988) and by Robert Davis and Carolyn Maher and staff. The task required these first graders to examine several groupings of blocks - a single small block, a stack of 4 blocks, a wall Of blocks that was 4 x 4 x 1, and a 4 x 4 cube — to respond to the question, How many of the small blocks will we need to make a big block? Make a drawing of your answer. The authors found that the cube problem produced more and better responses for three reasons. First, there were 29 less language demands on the children than with other problems. Second, the children were required to build a physical object (a familiar activity to first graders). Third, the time taken to build the model in order to answer the question may have allowed for increased reflection time. No other problem stimulated the amount or extent of discussion that resulted from the cube problem. The work of Cobb and Yackel ( 1994) took a different tack than that of Wilkinson and Martino (1993). Cobb and Yackel studied the development by second graders of mathematical argumentation. Yackel and Cobb (1994) grounded their study in current reform with its renewed emphasis on mathematical reasoning. In addition, their study drew on the work of mathematicians (i.e., Davis and Hersh, Lakatos, Balacheff) who have examined and written about the nature and purpose of proof. The study had a dual basis. First, teachers with whom they worked fostered an inquiry tradition which included social norms such as the development by students Of meaningful solutions, explaining, listening, asking questions, challenging explanations, justifying interpretations when challenged, and attempting to reach consensus on answers and solution methods. Second, these teachers fostered sociomathematical norms, or normative understandings - a shared understanding Of what to say, when, and to whom (p. 6). Cobb and Yackel wrote that sociomathematical norms are specific to children’s mathematical activity and regulate mathematical argumentation. Yackel and Cobb (1994) found that when they analyzed arguments several functions of arguments emerged. For example, some arguments functioned to force children to specify how they interpreted a problem or a solution method. 30 Other arguments functioned to convince others of an error (e. g., computation). Still other arguments functioned to nudge children’s thinking so that they made generalizations about thinking strategies. Lampert, Rittenhouse, and Crumbaugh (1996) discussed the challenges faced by students in disagreement. In their description of discussion in a fifth- grade classroom (whole group and small group), a more detailed perspective of frustration than described above by Corwin et. a1 is revealed: Experience of academic argument as an amicable mode of interaction in our culture is rare. . . The school classroom is a place where friends are made and lost, where identity is developed, where pride and shame and caring and hurting happen to kids. What they learn from social interaction cannot be described simply in terms of the mathematics covered by hashing out logical conflicts between various approaches to a problem. Mustering evidence to prove that an assertion is right or wrong is not a decontextualized learning activity. In the classroom, mathematical argument is done with and to the same people one plays with, eats lunch with, lives next door to, or has a crush on (Lampert, Rittenhouse, 8: Crumbaugh, 1996, p. 758, 759). The work by Lampert et. al (1996) goes to the heart of the challenge faced by teachers when disagreements arise: How do teachers manage the mathematics, the discourse, and disagreement while fostering participation norms intended to support increased mathematical power? And, how do they do all this so that children feel competent and respected (Ball 8: Wilson, 1996)? These are timely questions, questions as efforts to reform mathematics teaching and learning increase. They are also timely questions because they call to mind the usefulness and power in using sociocultural learning theory and sociolinguistics to increase understanding of the complexities of mathematical discourse from the child’s perspective. In the words Of Lampert et. al (1996), Talking about communication in conjunction with understanding is a radical idea. Understanding used to be thought of as a function Of 31 individual minds, and teaching and learning as transactions between the teacher and individual learners, even when there are 30 of them in the room at one time. But school people are beginning to take into account the social construction of knowledge, the relations between thought and language, and the importance of collaboration to real problem solving (p. 759-760). Lampert et. a1 (1996) recognized that studies Of individual understanding risk denying the social construction of students’ understandings. Studies which examine the relationship between social and intellectual aspects of mathematical disagreement are warranted. From a sociocultural learning perspective, studies of language use in learning shed light on students’ mathematical sense making. And sociolinguistics provides the analytical tools with which to get inside the meaning children make of mathematics content. Summary and Conclusion This study stands at the intersection Of sociocultural theory, sociolinguistics, and mathematical discourse in the context of reform — all of which appreciate language use. This is an exploratory and interpretive examination of student disagreements during whole-group discussion in a fourth-grade classroom. Recent studies of mathematical discourse have as their basis an appreciation of its complexity, as discussed above. In part, classroom discourse is so complex because it is unique to each setting. That is to say, classroom discourse (and disagreement) is constitutive of participants (teacher and student) and subject matter. It is also constitutive Of the meaning the participants construct in situ. It stands to reason, then, that the study of student disagreement would incorporate sociocultural learning theory as a theoretical framework and sociolinguistics as the methodological orientation. It is through 32 this dual lens that my study of students' disagreements in a fourth-grade mathematics class developed. 33 CHAPTER THREE METHOD With this chapter I share the ways in which this study evolved through my work with Katherine Vandenberg, an elementary teacher in a Professional Development School (PDS). In addition, I describe method and analyses in conjunction with this qualitative study Of student disagreement in a fourth-grade mathematics classroom. I spent a year in Mrs. Vandenberg's classroom as part of a larger study, and from this experience questions developed which led to my study Of student disagreement during mathematics. I first review the work of the larger study. I then describe the evolution of this research, describing the site, participants, data modes of Observation, field notes, interviews of the teacher and randomly selected students, uses of video and audio tape, and the information derived from spontaneous interactions with Mrs. Vandenberg and these students. In May, 1992, I began an assistantship at Edwards Elementary School, a PDS in a suburban community. My assistantship involved work with elementary teachers dedicated to thinking about and transforming their teaching of mathematics. In this collaborative research project, these teachers, Dr. Pamela Schram, a university faculty member, another graduate student, and I constituted the Mathematics Study Group Project, wherein we met to discuss, reflect on, and research the complexity of mathematics teaching and learning. My research interest in discourse during elementary mathematics classes mapped well onto the interest in discourse of these teachers. An August, 1992, interview with two Of the teachers, Pat Smith and Corrine Fox, confirmed that, indeed, this would be a good match. In September of that year, I participated in the second weekly meeting Of Edwards Elementary School's Math Study Group. As a PDS project group, we set about reiterating our anticipated goals. The teachers, Corrine Fox, Pat Smith, Felice Wilson, and Katherine Vandenberg discussed and elaborated their plans for the school year. As each teacher shared her vision, our work for the year began to unfold. For example, Corrine, a special education teacher, described her interest in thinking over the year around the theme, What's the point? In other words, Corrine wanted to reflect on, research, and write about why allowing students to discuss meaningful mathematical tasks was important to her. Pat, a first-grade teacher, discussed her interest in thinking about how students' contributions to the discourse facilitated ongoing assessment. Felice confirmed her interest, similar to Pat's, in thinking more and writing about assessment and the ways in which she thought about it. Finally, Mrs. Vandenberg spoke about her interest in the relationship between discourse and task in her classroom. More specifically, Mrs. Vandenberg expressed her desire to write about the ways in which the task affects the discourse. At that moment, I knew I was in partnership with a group of educators unique in that they investigated particular questions about their practice - questions arising from their experiences. Mrs. Vandenberg's focus, especially, seemed to parallel my interest in unpacking what goes on during mathematics classes when students are allowed and encouraged to discuss their ideas. As my work with the Math Study Group moved forward, Mrs. Vandenberg and I found ourselves sharing a common interest in students' contributions to the discourse, both spoken and written. Whereas Mrs. 35 Vandenberg asked herself how she could get everyone involved in discussions, I began asking, What was going on in these discussions, and who was involved? As I began observations in late September, sitting in the rocking chair at the rear, right corner of the room, I noted that there were more girls than boys in the class, 17 girls and 9 boys. One girl was Hispanic, and the rest Of the students were white.3 I learned that three children received Special Education services, and noted that three left during math time for Chapter I sessions. SO that I could gain some understanding Of Mrs. Vandenberg's teaching background and perspectives on learning, we targeted an hour to meet when possible during lunch or Math Study Group's scheduled weekly times. During these Fall sessions, I developed an increased appreciation of Mrs. Vandenberg's dedication to reflection on her own teaching with the goal of increasing students' mathematical understandings while enhancing her teaching of the subject. We discussed math classes I observed and my subsequent field notes, individual students who were and were not involved in the discussions, mathematical content and tasks, readings, university courses, and the complexity of teaching this way. One particular class from October 1 fascinated us both and prompted ongoing conversation for the rest of the school year. On this day in October, the topic was Odd and even numbers. Students were to discuss numbers they generated and tested the day before, with the goal of deciding if their numbers were Odd, even, or unknown (undecided). Mrs. Vandenberg and I Observed during the previous class discussion that two students, Tiffany and Deidra, disagreed about whether 70 was Odd or even. 3In January, another white girl joined the class; in April, an African American girl moved into the district and also joined the class. 36 Deidra said 70 was even and demonstrated that it divided into two numbers that were the same, 35 and 35. Tiffany argued that 70 was odd, because when you add 35 and 35, 5 and 5 is 10; carry the 1. One plus 3 plus 3 is seven. Seven's an Odd number so seventy is odd. In spite of subsequent and lengthy class discussion, Tiffany maintained an unorthodox stance that was to vex her for weeks: her belief that 70 was an odd number and that nobody else believed her but she thought 70 was an Odd number. So determined was Tiffany to convince others of her belief, she asked Mrs. Vandenberg, me, her parents, and classmates if we thought 70 was odd. Mrs. Vandenberg and I discussed this exchange and found ourselves focusing on the phenomenon of student disagreement and interested in using video tape as a means to capture teaching and learning. Thinking about this lesson raised questions for me especially. I asked: How do students disagree? What did Tiffany do to let her ideas be known? Why wasn't Tiffany‘ convinced that 70 was even? In the end, Mrs. Vandenberg and I wondered about the larger question: What is disagreement in this math class? In pursuit of data around these questions, I began video taping each lesson I observed, on the condition that as soon as it finished, I turned the tape over to Mrs. Vandenberg for her review. In the beginning, this was my primary reason for video taping: for Mrs. Vandenberg's use. As time passed, however, we both reviewed the video and it generated further questions and discussions about teaching and learning of mathematics. ‘Unfortunately, in early November, Tiffany moved to another community. 37 Site, Setting, and Participants This study took place in an elementary school on the eastern fringes of Hart, a suburban community south of a large, Midwestern university. Edwards Elementary School is part of the second largest school district in the area and is located 10 miles from the state capital. Hart was originally a farming community, but is rapidly becoming a suburb of the capital. It is primarily a residential community with little industry and few small businesses. A predominantly white community of approximately 11,750, Hart has a diverse socioeconomic population ranging from working class to middle income. At the time of the study, Edwards's student population of nearly 500 reflected the socioeconomic diversity of the Hart community. It also reflected the diversity of contemporary family forms: 54% Of its students lived with other than their biological parents. The student population included a large percentage who are considered at risk, as well as children who are average to above average students. Of Edwards' students, 18% received free or reduced lunch, and 6% received Special Education services. When I joined the Math Study Group, Edwards was in its third year as a Professional Development School. It was in this school that Mrs. Vandenberg was in her second year Of teaching. Whereas during the 1991-92 school year she taught second grade at Edwards, in 1992-93 she moved to fourth grade. In this, her first year as a fourth grade teacher, I met her in her room after school of the first full day. As members of Math Study Group, our work together began. Mrs. Vandenberg did not use the district’s fourth-grade math text, in part because her goal for the school year was to examine the relationship between math task and discourse. Instead, she opted to draw from multiple resources, primarily Marilyn Burns publications and TERC resources. For the geometry portion of the Fractions unit, she used Seeing Fragtiom (Corwin, Russell, and Tierney, 1990). This book was developed by TERC for the California Department of Education. In addition, she regularly conferred about resources, tasks, and specific lessons with Math Study Group colleagues, especially Pamela Schram, Project Director (who also taught in the room once a week). In addition to Mrs. Vandenberg, 25 of 26 children in her class were also participants in this study.5 Consent was not received for one of the boys. Therefore, although he was in two brief disagreements, his involvement is not discussed beyond the overall count of students in disagreement. M In this qualitative study, I examined the ways in which students in a regular fourth-grade class disagree during mathematics. I strove to make sense of student disagreement by drawing on existing Mathematics Study Group (MSG) Project data collected during 1992-1993. To increase understanding of these disagreements, a focal question and several sub-questions were developed to focus the study and analysis. The focal research question was: D. What is disagreement in this fourth-grade class? Sub-questions included: E. What do these students need to learn to be involved in disagreements? F. What are the roles of this teacher and these students during 5 Because this study was part of the larger Mathematics Study Group Project, parental and teacher consent for participation were received for that project. 39 disagreements? G. What is the relationship between disagreements and mathematical tasks? Drawing on the method of participant Observation (Erickson, 1986) for the larger MSG study, I gathered data from September, 1992, to July, 1993, from a fourth-grade class taught by Mrs. Vandenberg, the teacher assigned to this class. In Mrs. Vandenberg's classroom I Observed 36 mathematics discussions and made video and audio tapes of 35 lessons (see Appendix A). Initially, we did not plan to video tape; however, after my first day Of observation, I asked Mrs. Vandenberg if she would like to have video of what the children were saying. I suggested that I would tape lessons, and immediately remove the tape from the camera for her to take home that day. She agreed, and soon she asked me to watch these videos, also, so that I could reflect on what the children were saying. Initially, I recorded video for Mrs. Vandenberg, and wrote field notes for the MSG Project. My first goal was to help the children get comfortable with me, so Mrs. Vandenberg and I agreed that she would introduce me to the class. We also agreed that she would tell them that I was going to be in the room each week during math because I was interested in their math ideas. At the beginning of MSG data collection, Mrs. Vandenberg introduced me to the students and told the children that we were interested in how boys and girls learned math, and that I would be in the room each week to tape their math discussions. On each observation day, I interacted informally with the fourth graders before and after math time. In addition, when the children worked in small groups, I circulated around the room to observe their interactions and math journal writings. 4O When Mrs. Vandenberg agreed to video taping, I asked her if she would take a few minutes on my first recording day to introduce the camera, which she did. On that day when I first set up the video recorder, I also had the children wave to the camera and say hello. On subsequent days, the kids Often volunteered to help me set up or take down the camera, or asked me to join their group. Once, when Mrs. Vandenberg was called to the door, I continued class discussion for a few minutes until she returned (which, in the moment, indicated to us both how integrated I was into the setting). Throughout data collection, the children appeared comfortable in my presence, often sharing their journal entries with me and sometimes asking me questions (e.g., DO YOU think 70 is an odd or even number?) As part of the larger MSG Project data, Pamela Schram, Project Director, and I decided that before and after the fractions unit I would interview a group Of randomly selected students, in order to assess their learning Of fractions. Seven students were interviewed, about their fractions learning, and about their ideas on disagreement. I also spontaneously interviewed four additional students. We (members of MSG) intended these interviews to draw out kids' developing math thinking about fractions concepts. In the pre-fractions interview, I also asked open-ended questions about disagreement. For the purposes Of my dissertation study, then, the data included existing MSG Project video, field notes, and student journals. To triangulate the data, I examined student interviews, specifically the section on disagreement. I also conducted two formal interviews with Mrs. Vandenberg. Although we attempted to schedule these interviews during the fractions unit, we were limited by time constraints and end-of-year school demands. The focus of these interviews was 41 two-fold: to gain a sense of Mrs. Vandenberg’s professional development as a teacher of elementary mathematics and to learn about her beliefs about disagreement during mathematics discussions. In addition, Mrs. Vandenberg and I typically met briefly at the end of each math lesson to discuss mathematical tasks, issues raised by students, and student participation in discussions. Early on, when observing in Mrs. Vandenberg's room for the MSG study, I found myself in a researcher dilemma: how to take more or less extensive field notes while running the video camera. Because I Observed alone, there was no one to assist with one or the other. When I recorded field notes, I had to avert my eyes from watching classroom activity to the pages of notes. In my field notes I attempted to write as much detail as possible. For example, on October 1, a day early in data collection when I was learning children's names, at the beginning of math time I recorded the following details: 12:30 (Sts return to the room.) 12:35 (I sit to write after the camera is introduced.) KV: Boys and girls, I have something to tell you. Shhh. Right now the video has been running. . . Say hi to Mr. Vandenberg. sts: Hi Mr. Vandenberg KV: I really like the way Rob is ready to go. Can anybody tell, yesterday Tonya's and Moira's conjecture? Think back to what we were testing. (pause-no response from students) Think back to what Moira and Tonya proposed in class with the 10 numbers. What were we trying to find out. (pause) James, do you remember? James: NO. Micah: I have a headache. KV: You have a headache? I'm sorry to hear that Micah. 42 (Micah says something.) KV: Even odd numbers. (to all) How do you test numbers to find out if they're even or odd? Suzanne? (Suzanne responds.) KV: Suzanne said we used Unifix cubes or blocks and we divided them up and you were to record in your journal what you got. As a whole group I would like to give you the Opportunity to share what your results were. I have 2 charts. (moves to chalkboard in front of room, where 3 large posters are taped up.) Steve, would you predict what this (indicates the chart labeled, even.) chart is for? (Steve explains that it is for even numbers.) KV: What do you think will be in these 2 parts (indicates the columns she has drawn.) What these are for. (pause. NO student response) {Do they not understand the directions; or are they not yet attentive?) Think back to yesterday and what you did to test it. (DO they wonder what the it is?} (KV calls on a female st to explain. This female responds and explains. KV seeks clarification and understanding from students regarding what they should do with the chart. She calls on a male student; then Kim. Both students explain what they think they are to do.) I also have an unknown sheet. What would go there? Male st in back speaks quietly. KV called on the 2 students in front of room: You don't know if it's even or odd.) (KV then calls on a student in the back, who I think is the same one who spoke up. He says it means I'm not sure.) KV: Questions about what the chart is about? {Again, no response. Are students unsure Of directions? Need more connection with yesterday's work dividing up numbers? Were they unsure Of yesterday's task? I wonder. . .} (Students are instructed to record their numbers publicly and share.) KV: Who would like to share? (4 females and 2 males raise their hands.) Shawna? Shawna's going to tell us about the number she tested, the groups, and record it on the chart she thinks it fits into. {Were you 43 repeating instructions because you sensed some confusion on students part, or because you sensed this just was a day of sluggish thinking? Another reason?) DO you have a number in mind, Shawna? (Shawna goes to chart.) Shawna: I tell first, right? KV: What was your number? Shawna: I tested 80. I thought of 8, took away 4. . .(continues to explain her thinking about dividing up 80. Writes 40 + 40 = 80 in the first column on the even chart. This column was labeled, Number tested.) KV: She wrote 40 + 40 = 80. What was the number she tested? sts: 80. KV: And when she divided into groups what did she get? sts: 40 and 40. KV: Could we also write this (points to the 80) in the first column? In my field notes, I listened closely, to record as much as I could of Mrs. Vandenberg's questions and her revoicing Of kids' comments, and children's contributions, whether about the math topic on the floor or something else (such as Micah's headache). The fact that I could neither take adequate field notes nor record useful video if I continued to try to do both made me uneasy because I believe that much is learned from attending to the verbal and nonverbal communication during classroom discourse. And it was not acceptable to me to fluctuate between intense Observing and recording, fearing that while recording I would miss important goings-on. So much of classroom discourse is generative, building on what took place before, that I puzzled over an alternative that captured as much as possible, while recognizing that no method could get it all. Because I felt that the in situ Observing-recording-Observing cycle had the 44 somewhat limited potential to only get pieces of learning activity and lacked the continuity I wanted, I chose to capitalize on video technology to enhance my researcher eyes and mind. If something happened to catch my attention, I moved the camera toward it. Whenever I swept the room with my eyes, checking as a teacher does for things like students' level of attention to the task and activity or movement (e.g., a head lain down, a hand raised, a giggle, a walk to the pencil sharpener, a poke at a table mate, a grab for a math notebook), I also visually swept the room with the camera. In addition, as much as possible given the demands of my graduate assistantship and studies, I wrote field notes following observations. In this way, I believe I recorded much Of the complex and emergent nature Of the discourse. In the end, I found the camera work valuable in subsequent analyses. My analyses Of student disagreement drew from data collected during 10 lessons from April 28 to May 19, during a unit on fractions. Data collection proceeded with audio and video recordings of whole- and small-group discussion. In addition, data was collected from student mathematics journals, student and teacher interviews, Observation, and field notes. A1513 and mm recordings of whole- and small-group discussions provided a view and voice of participants. Student journals gave me an additional discursive window into student involvement in disagreements. Interviews allowed extended one-to-one interaction with participants which frequently led to insight into their perspectives of involvement in discussion and disagreement. My W and figlg goteg rounded out my data collection as a participant-observer, as I A documented the general setting in which mathematical disagreements emerged. 45 Analysis This was an exploratory study of disagreement during mathematics classes in one setting, and drew on sociolinguistic data analysis using the techniques of discourse analysis. The lessons analyzed are from a unit on fractions which began on April 28 and ended on June 11, 1993, with data from April 28 and May 19. Specifically, data is drawn from the following Observation dates: April 28, and May 3, 5, 6, 7, 10, 11, 12, 13, and 19. It should be pointed out that analyses are primarily of whole-group discussion, because the majority of discussions were in whole. To triangulate analyses, I also examined student math journals and interview data. I collected and photocopied math journals during the last week of school. I completed all interviews between April 22 and June 9. The purpose of analysis was to locate and examine disagreement. To address the research questions, my analyses began in Observation, for out of each observation, a pattern developed. First, before I entered the classroom for each observation, I had to consider what had transpired up to that point in combination with what Mrs. Vandenberg wanted me to focus on. In this sense, the ongoing evolution of classroom discourse and student’s involvement were always under analysis. Second, after each observation Mrs. Vandenberg and I usually discussed the math, expanding our sense of detail of students’ thinking. Third, over time, multiple waves Of analysis followed, based on the work of Shultz, Florio, and Erickson (1982). These analyses involved repeated viewing of mathematics lessons supported by field notes, student interviews, teacher interviews, and students' work recorded in their mathematics journals. First, I 46 searched for lessons with a high level of disagreement using indicators of student involvement such as increased pace, volume, and overlapping of student speech. By contrast, low-disagreement analyses examined pauses in speech and speaking turns, and slower pace, quiet voice, and lack of overlapping student speech. Second, I examined the mathematics in and around the disagreement; that is, which fractions tasks did or did not promote discourse and by association, disagreement? Additionally, I looked for norms of participation in disagreement to determine whether Mrs. Vandenberg prioritized consensus. This analysis helped me unpack the notion of disagreement, when and how it arose, the roles of the participants, the ways in which students used talk and writing during disagreements, and what students needed to learn to be involved in disagreements. To that end, analyses began in the setting during observation. As disagreement emerged in a class session (and when time permitted), Mrs. Vandenberg and I would confer while I took down the video camera, for the most part to begin thinking about why disagreement happened. The majority of my analyses, however, took place later, primarily through repeated viewing of video tape. To locate disagreement in the data, I viewed all lessons non-stop from beginning to end, in order to develop a disagreement rating (see Appendix B). This rating - either Sigh, medium or lgwd but especially focusing on high and low - led me to those lessons most likely to have disagreement. First, I searched for lessons with a high level of disagreement using indicators of student involvement such as increased pace, volume, and overlapping speech. By contrast, low-disagreement analyses examined pauses within and between and 47 speaking turns, slower pace Of speaking, quiet voice, lack of overlapping speech, and silence. During this wave of analysis, two lessons initially stood out due to the body language of the children: May 3 and May 7. On these two days, a Monday and Friday, the children seemed more physically active than on other days. Whether at the overhead or at their tables, their movement caught my eye. In order to examine these disagreements more closely and determine what the activity was about, I completely transcribed the video tapes from May 3 and May 7. These transcripts included as much detail as possible about the children’s use of nonverbal communication, so that I could draw on them for analyses. On further examination, I located in each lesson (May 3 and May 7) an episode of disagreement (the focus Of Chapter Six). Furthermore, to determine the extent of additional disagreement, I transcribed the remaining eight lessons, although not at the level of detail as the transcriptions of May 3 and 7. Further analyses revealed an additional 25 shorter disagreements, which ranged from two to nine turns (the focus of Chapter Five). In total, I located 25 single disagreements and 2 episodes of disagreement. Once a disagreement was located, I noted what preceded and followed it, including what Mrs. Vandenberg said or did immediately before and after disagreement. I then isolated the disagreement to study it rnicroanalytically. Then I replaced the disagreement to examine in its immediate context. After I had reconsidered both the disagreement and its context as a unit - how it arose, what happened during disagreement, and what followed - I repeated the process with consecutive disagreements. This process was repeated innumerable times. In addition, I looked across disagreements to detect patterns among them. 48 For example, when I located a brief disagreement early in math time on May 3, I extracted it, along with its immediately surrounding talk and noted that it was connected to the overhead question, What is a fraction? Then, I described it as follows: When whole-group discussion was underway, Monte said what a fraction was ’A fraction is, where you have a big number and you can't solve it so you split it up into groups so it's easier to solve. . . A fraction is a piece of the problem.’ While he talked he wrote for his example, ’200 + 10’ and drew several ’groups’ Of dots to represent his idea. Soon, Mrs. Vandenberg called on Hannah, who said ’200 + 10 is not a fraction,’ and began to explain that a fraction is less than one. During the course of her explanation, the following two-turn disagreement between Hannah and Monte emerged: KV: [200 + 10] Is NOT a fraction. But what you showed us is a fraction. Hannah: It's not a, it's NOT a fraction, because- Monte: But it CAN be a fraction. Hannaln because a FRACTION is less than ONE. TEN and TWO HUNDRED are not less than one. HERE is another kind Of fraction. There are other kinds of fractions. THIS would be, one-fourth, if you shaded in one. KV: OK. Hannah: And, KV: How did you know it would be one-fourth? Whereas Hannah was explaining her position on what a fraction was, Monte spoke up to re-state his different idea. In addition, I used a hypermedia data base to analyze data. My goal was to determine if patterns emerging from analyses existed in another setting. Use of Hypermedia in Analysis One layer of analysis perhaps unique to this study was my use of the M.A.T.H. Project's (Mathematics and Teaching Through Hypermedia, Deborah Ball and Magdalene Lampert, Co-Principal Investigators) data base to validate teacher moves that emerged during earlier analysis. This analysis took place during my involvement with a study group for teacher educators, Teacher 49 Educator Study Group (TESG).‘ Before TESG began, I decided that I would use my investigation there to play out dissertation questions and hunches. Drawn to students’ disagreements, I was intrigued when at our first meeting, Ruth Heaton, the group’s leader, played a June 1990, discussion (and disagreement) around 4/ 4 and 5/5. When I Observed this video I initiated an analysis of disagreement on June 7 (and others in the data base) and those from my dissertation work Here, on June 7, the third graders appeared to be disagreeing with one another, addressing one another's ideas, and, I think, using their own words to try to express in my mind important fractions thinking. For example, I remember at the October 5 session Of TESG Observing video and recording in my journal notes about the June 7 disagreement. In this disagreement, Lin argued that when comparing a cookie cut into fourths and a cookie cut into fifths, the shape of the cookie matters; that the shape of the cookies should be the same. In essence, Lin it's not the pieces or how much you shade in, it's that the cookies are the same shape. Mathematically, her argument was about the necessity of equivalence of shapes when comparing wholes under comparison. By contrast, Christina, who argued that 5/5 is more, appeared to have a surface view of the situation, by taking the stance that because Sis more than 4, 5/5 is more than 4 / 4. After all, she said, there are more pieces in 5/5. She did not seem to see the idea that Lin tried to articulate and illustrate. 6 The Teacher Educator Study Group was initiated in September, 1994, and met approximately two times per month to discuss issues of teacher education. Using the M.A.T.H. Project's hypermedia environments as a shared context, we regularly discussed mathematics teaching and learning in a third-grade classroom, and the investigations into mathematics teaching and learning by university preservice teachers. In addition, each TESG member developed an investigation Of third-graders' and preservice teachers' learning. We also regularly discussed these investigations (which we created in the form of an electronic notebook). 50 So, on October 5 at TESG I began by looking for disagreements early in the year, to see if there was evidence of how the teacher established the norms of participation in disagreement. I did not find a place where Ball, the third grade teacher, specifically said something akin to, ”When people disagree with one another you are to do X, Y, and Z.” I did find implicit ways in which Ball was nurturing the norms of disagreement. For example, I noted that at the beginning of the year on September 11, when students disagreed over whether Christina was 9 or 10 based on the year or year and date Of her birth, the talk appeared to go through Ball. That is, the children addressed their teacher rather than one another. Also, their manner of speech seemed more tentative, less assertive, as they spoke in quiet voices of what they were thinking. It was almost as though they may have been formulating their thoughts as they came from their mouths, as though an inward search for sense and whether the sense they were making mapped onto what they already knew. Sense-making was the term I recorded in my journal for what the children seemed to be doing as they participated in this particular disagreement. These two things, the fact that the students appeared to be disagreeing with one another and that the level of mathematics seemed more sophisticated at the end Of the year, seemed to set end-Of-year disagreement apart from what happened at the beginning of the year. Again, as I did in my analyses of the presence and absence of disagreement in my dissertation data, I started by looking where disagreement was and then where it was not, to better understand the notion Of the concept and its context. Over the next several weeks Of TESG, I continued thinking about the differences between disagreement on September 11 and on June 7. I already had 51 analytic notes asking whether Mrs. Vandenberg appeared to fuel, sustain, or extinguish disagreement and was calling them teacher moves. This was interesting to me to pursue because it compared the orchestration of disagreement by a novice, second-year teacher (Katherine Vandenberg) and Deborah Ball’s management Of it. Ball is a highly recognized teacher-researcher who chaired the Mathematics Teaching Working Group for the National Council of Teachers Of Mathematics, to develop the Professional Standgrgg fpr Tgaghing Mgthgmap’cs (1991). In other words, I began to ask if what Mrs. Vandenberg does in disagreement in the data may not be the isolated and idiosyncratic doings of one novice teacher. I asked whether what I found were recognizable teacher moves used by others who teach in the spirit of current mathematical reform. The final step in the multi-layered data analysis was to re-embed the disagreement in the larger context of what followed on May 3, as well as subsequent math lessons. I noted that the idea of 200 + 10 is a fraction re- surfaced later on May 3. When Joanne brought it up she volunteered that she was trying to make it into a fraction, by drawing a circle with 20 triangles. (She said, It’ s supposed to be 200 but I don’t know how to draw it.) Although Joanne’s circular representation Of 200 + 10, drawing 200 triangles with 10 colored in, was incorrect, it did indicate that she continued to think about whether and how it was a fraction. This example of Joanne’s re-emerging thinking, evidenced through her representation, was indicative of these students use of representations. Representations surfaced frequently as the children worked with their mathematical ideas during the fractions unit. 52 The location of such a micro-disagreement required attention to detail in order to isolate it. But it also demanded that I re-insert it in context so that I could connect it to the question on the floor. In addition, I attempted to connect disagreements over time, to determine if particular fractions concepts were more problematic than others for these fourth graders. Multi-layered analyses revealed that mathematical significance Of disagreement was connected to the task on the floor. As I will discuss in Chapter Five, some disagreements served different functions in the discourse. For example, some disagreements functioned to get at the underpinnings Of another’s assumptions and at the reasoning behind their position. Other disagreements functioned to publicly hold up two ideas for scrutiny. Still others functioned to pinpoint a surface flaw in a representation. In part, my rationale for studying disagreement was to analyze it from an end-of-year unit, because at that point in the academic year student disagreement was expected to be evident as a discourse move for which this teacher had planned for most of the year and, therefore, familiar to students. Second, bounding the study within the fractions unit allowed me to look closely at disagreement when content and lesson structure were similar while tasks differed. Third, what disagreement was and the norms for involvement in it should have been relatively stable during the time frame under analysis. It was reasonable to assume that at the end of the school year, these norms were familiar to the students. As my study unfolded, I became acutely aware of its complexity, because to my knowledge, there are no others like it from which I might draw or which I could duplicate. Therefore, to inform analysis, I drew on two areas of literature: 53 sociolinguistics, specifically the techniques of discourse analysis, and mathematical representations. Sociolinguistics informed the many layers Of analyses regarding discourse, disagreement and students' involvement in it. For these analyses, I drew on the work of Erickson (1986); Shultz, Florio, and Erickson (1982); Sacks, Schegloff, and Jefferson (1978); Gee, Michaels, and O’Connor, 1992); Cazden (1986; 1988); Edelsky (1993); O’Connor (1996); Hymes (1972); Tannen (1991) and); (Burbules, 1993). In my analysis of student disagreement during school mathematics I drew on the work of Lampert, Rittenhouse, and Crumbaugh (1996); Wilkinson and Martino (1993), and; Yackel and Cobb (1994). For analyses Of students’ mathematical representations, I drew on multiple writings by Ball (for example, 1990, 1996) and Lampert (for example, 1987). I anticipated learning from this analysis at least three things: a) a better understanding of what disagreement was, when it arose, how it was managed, and the role of participants; b) a sense of what students needed to learn to be involved in disagreement, and; c) the ways in which the existence of disagreement during mathematics classes enhanced (or didn’t) students' mathematical power. As analysis proceeded, I learned much more. 54 CHAPTER FOUR INSIDE DISAGREEMENT This chapter examines what Mrs. Vandenberg did to establish a discourse space in which her students can grapple with questions about fractions and, through discussion, challenge one another’ 5 thinking. Far from a generic entity, discussion within which disagreement was situated unfolded in an ebb and flow, with segments of one type or another. In this section I describe discussion patterns which emerged from data analyses. These patterns include two lesson formats generally followed by Mrs. Vandenberg, the discourse pattern of whole- group discussion, a typology of discussion, and, finally, categories Of disagreement. As I discuss below, Mrs. Vandenberg fostered a whole-group discussion pattern different from the traditional IRF (Sinclair and Coulthard, 197X), Initiation-Response-Feedback, where she revoiced (O’Connor, 1996) children’s comments and deflected them back into the discussion. When disagreement surfaced in whole-group discussions, she usually followed this same pattern. However, as described below, when extended disagreement occurred, this was not the case. I begin with a description of the two lesson formats. Lesson Format Mrs. Vandenberg tended to follow one of two formats for math time, which took place daily immediately following lunch recess and lasted for about one hour (12:30-1:30 pm.) In the first format, She began with an W W through which she oriented the students to the math focus for the day. In this segment, she typically provided an overview of the math problems, frequently in the form of questions, which she projected at the front of the room 55 from usual. their their jour gar dis d3 du V2 qll Va from an overhead projector (see Appendix C). During the second mpg, usually lasting 20-30 minutes, students (a) recorded the overhead problems in their math journals, (b) talked in small groups about the problems, and, (c) wrote their answers. Typically, when the children worked in small groups and with journals, Mrs. Vandenberg circulated around the room, interacting with individual children and small groups. She asked questions about their thinking and answers they recorded, reminded them of the task at hand, and generally gauged emergent thinking which she frequently drew on during whole-group discussion. In the Eli—IQ segment, Mrs. Vandenberg tended to lead whole-group discussion for about 20-30 minutes. Occasionally, she created journal questions during whole group, momentarily having the children reflect on and then further discuss an. issue raised that day. In the Logan segment, she concluded class and discussion either by reflecting and summarizing that day's discussion or by generating a journal question, which children recorded and spent a few minutes writing about. I also frequently observed a second format for math time, in which Mrs. Vandenberg started out with orienting comments, then moved immediately into whole-group discussion because the lesson was an extension or carryover from the previous day. Typically, she concluded whole group and presented new questions for the remainder of math time. In the case of the latter, once Mrs. Vandenberg posed new questions, the lesson structure looped back to resemble the first format. 56 Discourse Pattern Within these two lesson formats, whole-group discussion was characterized by a distinguishable discourse pattern that differed from the traditional IRF sequence of teacher-led discussion (see Appendix D). Over the course of a lesson, Mrs. Vandenberg’s pattern resulted in multiple points for children to get their ideas on the floor. As more and more ideas came to the floor, the potential also existed for student-to-student disagreement to occur and re-occur at different points across a lesson. In addition, she fostered a dialogical relation between people (Burbules, 1993) in the context of discussion, a relation ”to ’carry away’ its participants, to ’catch them up’ in an interaction that takes on a force and direction Of its own, Often leading them beyond any intended goal to new and unexpected insights” (p. 20). Following framing comments, during which she restated or posed the questions or problems for the day at the overhead projector, Mrs. Vandenberg generally Opened whole-group discussion by seeking comments from the children, with questions or comments such as: Who would like to be the first to share their idea about... What did you guys come up with? I'd like to hear some of your ideas about... Also, she reminded them about behavior or the task. For example, she would say something like: Open your journals and also take notes. You should be writing down 57 When someone is up here sharing their ideas and we're calling out or talking, that shows them we're not interested in their idea. That's not acceptable. Right now you're supposed to be These insertions served to index (Mehan, 1979) a rule about respect for others - respect for their ideas and about how to talk and listen to each other. When combined, orientation to the lesson, overhead questions for the day, and questions and comments to start getting ideas on the floor ultimately launched the overall process of getting children’s turns and ideas into the discussion. Therefore, when combined, orientation to the lesson, overhead questions for the day, and questions and comments to start getting ideas on the floor ultimately launched the overall process of getting children’s turns and ideas into the discussion. After discussion was underway, Mrs. Vandenberg frequently repeated what children said and then said or asked things which appeared to seek clarification. As wave after wave Of student responses came to the floor, she revoiced (O’Connor 8: Michaels, 1993) them and then said things such as: Can you draw (or write) that? Tell us about your picture. How do you know? What do you mean, ? Mrs. Vandenberg frequently repeated what a child said back to the group. But sometimes, to the individual explaining an idea, she tended to ask questions which invited even more ideas (and potentially disagreement). As more and more of the children’s thinking went through her, whether intentionally or not, 58 she gave ideas credence through the way in which she either repeated or synthesized them: Why does that make sense? Do you guys agree? What do you guys think? ....Monte responded to the question, 'What is a fraction?', by writing this DIVISION7 problem. Any responses? To further stir the stew of different ideas she brought to the public forum of whole-group discussion, Mrs. Vandenberg made connecting comments among children’s ideas, noted her confusion about (apparently) conflicting statements, or asked questions. To do the first, connect ideas, she would say, Hannah's last comment was that that was FOUR-fourths. And Ali said it was ONE, because it was one whole.. (to Micah, about Antonia's halves on the geoboard) Good. One had seven and a half, one had eight and a half. 50 MIKE showed us how if we just switched that half we could have eight and eight. SO it looks like ANTONIA thought about this too. Braden, what did YOU want to say? To accomplish the second, she might say, ”I'm confused about something,” then go on to restate two different ideas the children had previously talked about, either that day or another day. For example, on May 13 during the discussion of the placement of 1 / 2 on the number line, Jody insisted that 1/ 2 wouldn't be between zero and one - it would be somewhere else. When Steve followed by saying 1 / 2 would be between 2 and 3 on the number line, Mrs. Vandenberg said, ”I'm confused about something," and went on to explain that the day before they had discussed and written in their journals that 1/ 2 + 1/ 2 = 59 1; but that day (May 13) they were saying 1/ 2 was between 1 and 2, or between 2 and 3. This seemed to hold more than one idea up for scrutiny. To get more ideas on the floor, Mrs. Vandenberg frequently asked questions. Within this discourse pattern with ideas coming to the floor from the beginning of whole-group discussion to its conclusion, the potential for disagreement existed almost from the start. Disagreements did not always occur, but inherent in this discourse pattern - with more and more ideas coming to the floor - children had the Opportunity to disagree. In fact, disagreement bubbled up as early as 2 minutes into one lesson and as late as 51 minutes into another. When disagreement-like talk began to surface, Mrs. Vandenberg often played an active role in orchestrating and mediating the interaction. She inserted questions and comments as the disagreement emerged, so that the turn-taking pattern became teacher-student-teacher-student. For example, early in math time on May 7, Michaela shared her way to make halves on the geoboard, an overhead question for May 5 (see Appendix C). Because her design raised two controversial ideas, disagreement began to emerge. First, as shown in Figure 1, Michaela’s shapes did not use all Of the geoboard space, nor the center peg. Referring to the number of pegs within each shape, she described what she did to make them equal. Michaela: Then there’s, 12 in there. And then here’s another 12 and you’ve got one nail left over. SO I took THAT nail and I made up nails with rubber BANDS to make it EQUALLY. . . 7 Words in call capital letters indicate speaker emphasis. Commas within speaker turn indicate brief pauses. Vertical dots indicate missing transcript. 60 Second, Michaela said she needed to use the center peg, so she put a lone band over it, while she also laid other bands down the uncovered portion of the board. After she said she was ”pretending those are nails,” a teacher-student- teacher-student pattern of teacher mediated disagreement emerged. Tomika expressed concern, which Mrs. Vandenberg repeated and asked about: Tomika: I don’t THINK so. KV: You don’t think so? Why don’t you think so? Tomika: Well when she put those two rubber bands in, like, like there. ’Cause you can’t do it. KV: Tomika doesn’t understand or agree with the rubber bands in the middle part. Michaela: Well if there IS a nail you make it EQUALLY. SO I made a part with rubber bands. By taking up the children’s dissenting remarks in this way, the disagreement went through Mrs. Vandenberg, rather than directly to Michaela. However, at times the children would directly uptake one another to speak for themselves, resulting in short bursts of student-student disagreement. Figure 1: Michaela's Way to Make Half on May 7 Teacher Moves As described above, Mrs. Vandenberg generally followed a pattern during whole-group discussion. Wthin this pattern, she made particular moves to 61 nudge discussion along. These moves included: a) asking questions, b) connecting ideas, c) getting children to say more, and, c) inserting reminders about task and behavior. Asking mestiops When Mrs. Vandenberg orchestrated the discourse by asking questions, common questions she used included What do you guys think about that? What did you come up with? Can you Show us that? How did you know? What do you mean by _? DO you agree? nnectin Ideas an Gettin hi1 ren t a Mor Second, Mrs. Vandenberg frequently connected kids' ideas, for example, ”Hannah said 4 / 4. Ali said one.” or ”I'm confused about something.” Then she went on to compare two different things about the same issue, like whether 1 / 2 was less than 1. Or, she would make a specific connection such as, ”That reminds me of what ALI showed us. DO you remember how ALI showed us her way of dividing up circles?” Mrs. Vandenberg also said things to get kids to say more, such as ”Tell us about _,” or ”Explain what you mean by _.” Behavior and Task Reminderg She also inserted behavior and task-related reminders as each day's discourse unfolded, as in the following example from April 28: I'd like you to Open your journal and also take notes about what is being shared now. That's not appropriate to call out. Shhh. Boys and girls stop 62 and listen. When someone's up here sharing their ideas and we're calling out or talking to our table mates that shows them that we're not interested in their ideas. That's not acceptable. Right now you're supposed to be taking notes on these ideas we're generating and listening. . . Shhh. It's really hard to hear people's ideas. You all have such wonderful ideas it's disappointing when we can't hear some people. Especially some Of you who have been waiting a while to share your idea. Insertions such as this one revealed that Mrs. Vandenberg did not take for granted that the children knew how to be involved in discussions. Instead, this was an indication of the work she did throughout the year to nurture discussion norms, affording the children some direct instruction about how to be involved. Because of these insertions, the majority of disagreements were over the math content, not interpersonal conflict, or something like shooting bands across the room (the reason for a lone non-math disagreement). When combined, Mrs. Vandenberg’s questions, connections, and reminders helped to keep the children focused on the mathematics while they explored their developing ideas and how to reason about them. However, the discourse pattern and Mrs. Vandenberg’s teacher moves within it as discussed above did not result in one type of discussion. To the contrary, analyses revealed a typology of discussion across the data. Typology of Discussion These discussions took on various nuances, and analyses of them led me to an examination of disagreement. Indeed, four types of discussion emerged from the data: repetitive discussions; discussions with different ideas, without disagreement; discussions with different ideas, with teacher-mediated disagreement, and; student-to—student disagreement. 63 Ii— Repgtitive Disgpgsippg Some discussions were flat and difficult to attend to. I sat, awaiting the breakout of a disagreement, eager to witness how it would begin and what it would be about. But agreement, not disagreement, marked these discussions. With the same or similar ideas repeated by students, these repetitive discussions did not appear to go anywhere. They retraced similar notions, and if discussions were represented as lines, these would be flat. A small-group discussion on April 28 illuminates the notion of repetitive discussion. On this, the first day of the fractions unit, Mrs. Vandenberg followed the first lesson format, beginning with orienting comments, then questions for the day on the overhead projector: What is a fraction? When have you used fractions? How are fractions represented? Draw and label pictures. She read the questions to the class and instructed students to discuss and brainstorm the questions in small group with their table mates. They were to draw and label pictures as a group on a large white sheet of paper. After about five minutes of math time the camera was moved to capture the first small group's interaction. This group of four consisted Of three girls and one boy: Connie, Moira, Suzanne, and Monte. A microphone in the middle of the table captured their talk. They soon were at work on the first question: What is a fraction? Nearly 6 minutes into their work, Moira suggested that a fraction is when you ”cut up into pieces.” Monte quickly added, ”it's when you split up a problem and make it easier,” which he soon repeated. Connie turned to the camera to say ”we're talking about fractions today,” while Suzanne said nothing about the question. Her contributions were in the form of talking to and giggling with Moira. With the large sheet in front of her, Moira spoke aloud as she recorded, ”A fraction is when you split something up”, to which the others said, ”OK.” The original ideas shared by Moira and Monte, then, were that a fraction is when you cut something up or split up a problem and make it easier, which Moira recorded as ”A fraction is when you split something up.” To them, cutting or splitting something up was associated with fractions. These contributions were basically the same and did not differ or provoke disagreement. They were recorded on the group paper and the group turned to the second question, When have you used fractions .7, as the camera moved on to the second small group. Discussions With Different Ideas. Without Disagreement On occasion, Mrs. Vandenberg asked for students' ideas about a mathematics issue or tOpic under consideration, and she got them. However, students did not disagree with one another; nor was she able to inspire a disagreement. The different ideas hung in the air without reaction or response. I labeled these discussions with diflerent ideas, without disagreement. Frequently, I wondered if the children did not notice that different ideas were coming to the floor. Mrs. Vandenberg would say something to highlight these differences and still there would be no student reaction. For example, on May 4, the children worked for the first time with geoboards on the following task, recorded at the overhead: 65 How many ways can you show halves (1 / 2)? Practice on geoboards. Record on paper. Write one explanation in journal, explaining how you can prove your idea shows a half. When Mrs. Vandenberg introduced the task, she had a few children go to the overhead to begin showing their ways to make halves. When Hannah cut her drawing“ into fourths, Mrs. Vandenberg asked how she could prove she had a half. Hannah’s proof was that on one side she had 2 pieces, and ”Four take away two is two. ’Cause two plus two is four. It’s a half." Others, like Moira, who made a diagonal cut, said ”They’re the same size so each kid would get it equally." And, Jody called out that a line down the middle of the board ”has the same amount Of spots." Later, when Ali showed her different idea (as she said, her ”odd one”) about halves (see Figure 2, Ali’s Halves on May 4), Mrs. Vandenberg tried to no avail to fuel disagreement. At the overhead, Ali counted pegs to prove she had halves, and said she had 9 pegs on each side. Apparently noting that these halves were not equal, Mrs. Vandenberg asked, KV: If that was a brownie and you had cut that for me and a friend, I might say that that’ s not fair because I didn’t get the same size piece. Ali’s counting pegs. DO you guys agree? DO you guys agree that there’s 9-that that shows equal halfs? In spite of these strategic questions to direct the children’s attention to the discrepancy in Ali’s reasoning, between counting pegs to get halves when she actually did not end up with equal halves, the children were unresponsive. 8 Note that Hannah did not use a geoboard to represent her example. Instead, she drew a square, calling it a ”brownie." 66 Figure 2: Ali’s Halves on May 4 In discussion segments like the one above, when Mrs. Vandenberg persisted, even asking if anyone disagreed, students remained unresponsive. It is not possible to say whether, in spite of Mrs. Vandenberg's efforts, they did not recognize the differences for themselves, were out of ideas, or were not interested. Or, as in the case above, the children may simply not have had enough time so early in the lesson to think of additional ways to make halves. Whatever the reasons, the result was that students did take up Ali’s idea to disagree. Discussions With Different Ideas, Teacher-Mediated Diaagpagment At times the discussions became discussions with diflerent ideas, with teacher- mediated disagreement, for example when Tomika’s disagreement on May 7 went through Mrs. Vandenberg, or when disagreement over Michaela’s unique way to use extra bands and ”make up nails” to divide the board in half. I found these segments of disagreement to be characterized in several ways, first by the fact that Mrs. Vandenberg mediated them. In a predominantly teacher-student- teacher-student pattern, kids' comments went through Mrs. Vandenberg. She sought student responses, they talked to her, she restated the idea, another student answered, and so on. Second, the students disagreed for the most part with each other's ideas, rather than the person - a participation norm endorsed 67 by Mrs. Vandenberg. In other words, when Mrs. Vandenberg mediated, the disagreement tended to elevate the mathematical aspects of the discussion. When children began to veer from a focus on ideas or toward inappropriate interactions, Mrs. Vandenberg often stepped in with a management comment, a third characteristic Of teacher-mediated disagreement. The following exchange from April 28 demonstrates Mrs. Vandenberg's involvement through teacher-student turn-taking, her conversational work to get students to focus on ideas, and her move to manage unacceptable behavior. After small-group time to work on overhead questions, she began whole-group discussion about the question, What is a fraction? KV: By now you’ve generated ideas about what you think fractions are. I’d like to hear some of those different ideas about what you came up with. Let’ 3 start with the first question: What is a fraction? What did you come up with? St: Stop kicking me! Who’s kicking me? KV: I’d like you to Open your journal and also take notes about what is being shared now. I think that’ 11 help you focus. This immediate interjection to remind students about both behavior and journal writing quickly turned attention to ideas that came to the floor. Student-t9_-Student Disaggeement The fourth discussion type, which is the focus of this study, is student-to- student disagreement. The predominant characteristic of these disagreement- based discussions was the idea that the children were disagreeing with one another. That is to say, there was a mutually engaging aspect of communication with one student taking up what another said, and disagreeing with it. These disagreement-based discussion segments, similar to teacher-mediated 68 I_ Vi disagreement, appeared generative to the overall discussion by drawing in multiple ideas over which the children interacted. These discussion segments differed from those where different ideas were on the floor but not acted upon, and differed from those where disagreement filtered through Mrs. Vandenberg. Mrs. Vandenberg and I discussed disagreements, Often right after math time when students were preparing for Writing Workshop. We informally attempted to tease apart the disagreements to try to figure out why some struck “8 as good, or constructive, ones. Intrigued by their mathematical content or the twists disagreements brought to the discussion, or who was involved in them and in what way, we found the disagreements strategic in our efforts to understand students' mathematical learnings. Once during the fractions unit the disagreement took a negative turn. This disagreement was unlike any other in the data. As I will discuss in Chapter Six, the mathematics under discussion was overshadowed by the negative interactions of some children. Here, students' different ideas arose and were disagreed with. However, the difference is that the mathematical aspect Of the disagreement was not elevated; the interactional aspect was. If one could imagine a balance scale on which one side held the weight of the mathematics in the disagreement and the other the interactional, the scales in that instance would be tipped toward the latter. To summarize, the lessons format, whole-group discourse pattern, and discussion types led me to a closer examination of the discursive phenomenon of disagreement. In the next section, I define disagreement in this setting and compare it to the more generic notion of argument. I begin with a definition of disagreement. 69 What is Disagreement? If discussions took on differing characteristics, some of which resulted in disagreement, what then was disagreement in this setting? To Mrs. Vandenberg, in part it was about having and sharing different ideas, as evidenced in both her questions of the day (at the overhead) and particular questions she asked children. However, there was a difference here between sharing ideas and disagreement. When the sharing of ideas was going on, multiple students offered various answers or strategies and Mrs. Vandenberg orchestrated discourse with questions and comments. And when disagreement happened, one or more students would take up the comment Of another, to distinguish their idea. For example, on May 3, Mrs. Vandenberg solicited students ideas about the discussion question, What is a fraction? Responses included: 200 divided by 10 is a fraction. (Monte) A fraction is kind Of like division. (Joanne) A fraction is less than 1. (Hannah) Although similar, the children apparently perceived them to be different because they talked to their teacher, not a classmate, about them. They did not directly uptake one another's contributions; instead, when called on, they directed their comments to Mrs. Vandenberg. Without immediate uptake to link the previous student's idea, it was not disagreement for the purposes of this study. In these examples, students' ideas went through the teacher who repeated them, in essence deflecting them back to the children. They appeared to be addressed to her, she took them up, and rephrased them. For the purposes of this study, disagreement is defined as those instances where (a) a student had an 70 idea, (b) a second student had a different idea, and (c) the second student directed it back to the first. This is when students negotiated the floor for themselves and assumed responsibility both for getting a turn and their idea onto the floor. They initiated their own turn to disagree with a particular person around a previously-stated idea. With 26 active fourth graders and Mrs. Vandenberg daily engaged in the construction Of mathematical meaning and social norms, disagreement was a mutually engaging undertaking, where one student contributed an idea to the discussion and others reacted and responded to it. A statement was made and locked onto by another. Interestingly, disagreement parameters were not openly stated by the teacher until late in the hour on May 7: KV: Let's remember that we're disagreeing with each other's ideas, not the peOple. Okay? To raise your hand to someone else or, to, yell at someone else is not appropriate. With this brief and straightforward reminder to her students, Mrs. Vandenberg appeared to bring to focus the participation structure (i.e., the mutual rights and obligations of students, Erickson, 1996) during disagreement. That is, Speaker's right: to disagree Others' duty: to listen Speaker's duty: disagree with ideas Others' right: to expect they will not be personally attacked Here, the norms of participation in disagreement appeared vivid: It was appropriate to disagree with ideas rather than people, and inappropriate to raise a hand or to yell at someone. Mrs. Vandenberg made a distinction between 71 .sagreeing with ideas and disagreeing with classmates, separating the Iathematical and interactional aspects of disagreement. Throughout the data set, Mrs. Vandenberg appeared to hold no xpectation that students would come to consensus, because she never pecifically stated that. Rather, the sharing of ideas often appeared to be the ;Oal. For example, on countless occasions Mrs. Vandenberg would say something akin to the following: KV: . . . a FEW groups didn't get to SHARE one of their ideas yet. Monte, would you like to share what YOU came up with for what is a fraction? This sharing of ideas seemed important to her desire to develop tasks which were conducive to discussion, a desire she expressed during a September 26 meeting, when she said the question she wanted to focus on for the year’ 5 Math Study Group work was, What is the relationship between the task and discussion? In other words, to her the quality of discussion depended on the mathematical tasks she developed. And, an important aspect of discussion was sharing ideas. Curious about Mrs. Vandenberg’s frequent use of ”share,” I asked her the origin of the language of share, sharing, and share your idea. After a pause, she said that it may have come from when she taught second grade, when she thought ”Show and Tell” did not capture the interaction for which she hoped. She changed to ”share” then because she thought "Show" and ”showing” doesn't have the social piece. Sharing is more like welcoming feedback or input (field notes, February 21, 1994). When I asked if, when she used ”share” and ”sharing,” she was thinking mathematically, socially, or both, she answered, ”Socially. Thinking aloud with the group; for example, a question, a 72 representation, or a different idea” (field notes, February 21, 1994). Through this and other responses, I gained insight into the priority Mrs. Vandenberg placed on getting children to talk and interact around their mathematical ideas about fractions content. Mrs. Vandenberg also elaborated on the importance of disagreement during discussions. During a July interview, I asked her why she thought disagreement was important KV: Well I think the disagreements part is important because I think, WHEN disagreements occur I think that, that FORCES learners to, ARTTCULATE what they're thinking about, and to, to JUSTIFY, how, how they SOLVE something or why they THOUGHT about something in a particular way. If there's NOT disagreements and things are just ACCEPTED as, ’OK’, you know and you move ON, then I don't really think we're forcing them to stretch their thinking and to consider different alternatives and, um. And then I think a big part Of disagreement, ALSO is accepting one another's ideas and, seeing the value, in ah, in different ways Of thinking. Which, which I think is something that takes a lot of WORK, in the classroom. Um, you know just, just getting children to, to listen to one another and want to RESPOND to each other and not always to the teacher. (July 2, 1993) Although she did not intend disagreement to be her focus, it did become an interest over time, as she worked to incorporate discussion into math lessons during this, her first year of teaching fourth grade. This sense Of interaction through sharing ideas, of children talking with each other, emerged again and again during discussion. To this teacher and these fourth graders, disagreement was a more immediate, face-to-face process than the more formal notion of argument (Toulmin, 1958). Rather than a final- draft, refined and structured means to present or refute a position, disagreement to these kids occurred in the moment through exploratory talk (Barnes, 1992), sometimes as the ideas seemed to come to them in the moment. And the longer 73 the disagreement, the more personal it became, as I will discuss in Chapter Six. In this setting, there was more to disagreement than math, because in disagreement, the children took opportunities to get both a turn and an idea on the floor. Categories of Disagreement My review Of the data resulted in identification of two types of disagreement, single disagreement and episodes of disagreement. Although virtually all disagreement involved fractions content, single disagreement and episodes of disagreement differed in several ways. First they differed in length. In the shorter disagreements, which I labeled single disagreement, the number of turns ranged from two (May 3) to nine (May 12). These disagreements were bounded by either questions or comments from Mrs. Vandenberg. For example, on May 13, part Of whole-group discussion was about the question, Where is 1/2 on the number line? Mrs. Vandenberg called on Antonia to show where she put 1 / 2. Antonia first made a mark between zero and 1, saying it was ”in the middle of zero and one”: KV: What made you decide that? Antonia: I don’t know. Or else it could be here. (marked between 0 and - 1) Jody: But that’s a NEGATIVE number you’re going by! KV: Wait a minute! A couple Of people came up to ask me things. But I prefer that you raise your hand. Antonia, why did you put it between zero and 1? In this exchange, Mrs. Vandenberg’s questions required Antonia to explain her thinking, which she seemed unable to do. But as soon as she marked he spot halfway between zero and negative one, Jody called out her Opposition 74 to the idea. However, after a management reminder, Mrs. Vandenberg stayed with Antonia, bounding this disagreement (which will be further discussed in Chapter Five) with her question. This bounding helped keep the focus on the math under discussion. The second characteristic of single disagreement was the uptake by the children of Mrs. Vandenberg’s questions and comments. In the above example, although Antonia did not answer Mrs. Vandenberg, Jody continued to protest, this time to her teacher. In this way the children’s attention was directed to the idea on the floor: the location of 1 / 2 on the number line. To summarize, single disagreements were identifiable first by length: They varied between 2 and 9 turns. Second, following these short bursts of student-tO-student disagreement, Mrs. Vandenberg successfully re-negotiated her way to the floor and continued to orchestrate the discussion. Third, the children responded to Mrs. Vandenberg and take up her questions or comments to answer her. In this cycle - teacher question, student-to-student disagreement, teacher question - this teacher and these fourth graders mutually negotiated the floor. However, there were longer and consecutive waves of disagreement during which Mrs. Vandenberg receded from the talk and the children appeared tO take over. In these more extended instances, or episodes of disagreement, the :hildren either did not hear or ignored Mrs. Vandenberg, and she had to work at 'enegotiating the floor. It was as though the disagreement took on a life of its Iwn (Burbules, 1993), and whereas the majority of shorter, or single, lisagreements were over the math content on the floor, in episodes the focus on 1ath seemed to fade. Rather than the math in the disagreement being 75 highlighted, in the episodes, the interactions between the children were what stood out. It is important to point out that there is an instance of single disagreement on May 12 (discussed in Chapter Five) that at first glance may appear to be an episode. However, the distinguishing feature in this 9-turn disagreement is the fact that when Mrs. Vandenberg attempted to regain the floor, she succeeded. As I will point out in Chapter Six, this was not the case during episodes. There, for some time the children did not respond to Mrs. Vandenberg’s questions, instead continuing to disagree. 76 CHAPTER FIVE SINGLE DISAGREEMENT The disagreements discussed in this chapter concerned fractions content, Ich as part-whole relationships, equivalence, number and size of pieces Of the 'hOle, whether to count pegs or squares on the geoboard, and the location of a raction on the number line. As defined in the previous chapter, single lisagreement is a brief uptake of one student by another, to get a different but 'elated point on the floor. Of the identified 25 disagreements of this type, 24 were mathematical by nature. That is, these disagreements brought to the floor the children’s developing fractions ideas. I learned that, as these children teased apart this mathematics content, they relied on representations to explore their developing knowledge. And, I found that an examination of disagreements provided a window into students’ emergent thinking about fractions. Representations were a catalyst for most of the disagreements, because they helped the children focus on either the mathematical idea being represented or how a classmate was thinking about and representing that mathematical idea. Mrs. Vandenberg started whole-group discussion, brought ideas to the floor and representations with them, and disagreement erupted. Sometimes, the disagreement was about an underlying mathematical idea depicted by the representation, and sometimes the disagreement was about the construction of the representation itself. Two equally important competencies were at work when these fourth graders disagreed: academic competence and social competence (Mehan, 1979). These fourth graders had to be involved in the math content, or academically competent, in order to know about what to disagree. And, they had to be socially competent to know how to disagree. One competency was not sufficient. With one and not the other - academic or social competence — they did not successfully negotiate the floor with a turn and an idea. For example, if a student knew her fractions content but not the appropriate juncture or way to disagree, she likely would not successfully get her disagreement to the public forum of whole-group discussion. Inversely, if he knew the appropriate juncture at which to negotiate the floor and get a different idea to the public forum of whole—group discussion, if he did not know the math, he may not succeed. When a child could demonstrate both, she was more likely to succeed. There are three ways to parse these disagreements: by fractions content, by purpose, and by function. The examination of content revealed that these fourth graders disagreed over that content which we would anticipate in a unit on fractions, such as equivalence, part-whole relationships, area, and the location of a fraction on the number line. The disagreements also differed in purpose - they could, for example, correct, defend, or question assumptions of another. Further, some purposes of disagreement were similar, such as disagreement to correct, to revise, and to mediate (as indicated in Table 1). Together, these purposes Of disagreement — to correct, to revise, or to mediate — served a function at a general level in the discourse. Disagreements of these purposes functioned 0 either pinpointed a flaw in what a child said about a representation or linpointed a flaw in a representation as created. As shown in Table 1, the 24 )ntent-related disagreements fell into six categories of purpose which are the ganizing frame to this chapter: Correct, Revise, Mediate, Defend, Question Iother’s Assumptions, and Call for Clarification. These categories of purpose 1 be consolidated to reveal the functions of disagreements. The functions of 78 these disagreements in turn identified the mathematical significance of the disagreements. To demonstrate the purposes, functions, and mathematical significance of the disagreements, I include in each section examples of children’s disagreements. I begin with the first category of purpose, Disagreement to Correct, Revise, or Mediate. 79 Table 1: Purposes Of Disagreement Grouped by Function 9Purpose Function Representations Correct Another Pinpoint a flaw in what is Joanne’s circle drawing (n=7) said about a of 200 + 10 representation Circle representation with overhead question, Is this 1/2, 2/4, or both? Why? Rob’s geoboard of 1/ 2 and 2 / 4 Monica’s drawing of 3/5 (2x) Jonah’s geoboard Drawing of 1 / 2 Revise Pinpoint a flaw in a Joanne s r sctangle (n=2) representation as created drawmg Fraction bars Mediate Link 2 different ideas Circle representation (n=1) with overhead question, Is this 1/2, 2/4, or both? Why? Defend Hold up 2 ideas for 200 + 10 is not/ can be a (n=6) scrutiny fraction Monica’s drawing of 3/5 Kim’s diamond Rob’s geoboard of 1 / 2 and 2/ 4 Question Get at underpinnings of Hannah’s 2 / 4 drawing Assumptions another’s position to represent 1/ 2 (n=7) Hannah’s TV Number Line Call for Clarification Get at reasoning behind Kim’s Diamond (n=1) another’s position 8O Correct, Revise, or Mediate Nine disagreements had as their purpose either to revise or correct someone else. Essentially, these disagreements were to correct some aspect of a representation, either to correct a discrepancy between what one said and what one created as a representation or to correct the representation as created. In addition, one disagreement’s purpose was to mediate two opposing ideas. Disagreements to correct or revise functioned to pinpoint a flaw in a representation or what was said about a representation. These disagreements focused on a specific point. For example, on May 3, Joanne drew one thing and said another, resulting in a brief disagreement. Joanne attempted to represent Monte's idea, ”200 + 10 is a fraction,” as a circle. This notion about fractions came up in response to the overhead questions for math time (Day 2 of the Fractions Unit) on May 3 (and which were carried over from April 28): What is a fraction? When have you used fractions? How are fractions represented? Draw and label pictures. Early in that lesson, Mrs. Vandenberg started whole-group discussion by asking Monte, ”Monte, what did you think a fraction was? How did you describe it?” Monte answered ”A fraction is, where you have a big number and you can't solve it so you split it up into groups so it's easier to solve . . A fraction is a piece of the problem." He went on to say that ”200 + 10 is a fraction. ” Monte recorded circled groups Of 10 dots as his representation for 200 o- 10, and later in the hour when Monte's idea again surfaced, Joanne worked at :reating a circle to represent it. She worked to divide up her circle, in her word, nto ”triangles," so that her drawing looked like a pie cut into many pieces: 81 KV: So we're assuming that's 200 and you're pretending to color in 10 Of them? Joanne: Yes. I'm putting 10 triangles down and colored 5. Five goes into 10. Ten goes into 200. st: you only have 9. Joanne: Nuh uh. 1, 2, 3, 4, 5, 6, 7, 8, 9. (colors in one more "triangle") KV: Nobody has an idea about this 200 divided by 10? I think we should come back to that. In the process of working through her picture, Joanne said she didn't know how to make 200 pieces, so we had to pretend, and that she colored in 10. When a classmate said she colored only 9, she initially disagreed, but then colored in a tenth ”triangle." Her action seemed to indicate her agreement. She assumed she'd colored 10 triangles, but learned she had not. The mismatch between what she colored and what she said did not alter the correctness of what she was trying to represent. A second example of correcting another’s representation occurred on May 4 , the first day the children worked with geoboards, when Jody corrected Jonah's representation of one-half Of the geoboard. ‘*l 1 O O O i .__._.l 1 O O O 1 A A A Figure 3: Jonah's Way to Show One-half on May 4 'o begin Class, Mrs. Vandenberg recorded these directions on the overhead: How many ways can you Show halves (1 / 2)? Practice on geoboards. Record on paper. 82 Write one explanation in journal, explaining how you can prove your idea shows a half. More specifically, she passed out geoboards and bands as well as dot paper. The children practiced making halves on the board, re-created shapes on the paper, then explained in their journals. Jonah was first to volunteer to show a way at the overhead, and when his horizontal line didn't connect all the dots (representing geoboard pegs), Jody corrected him: KV: Who can come up here and show me one way I could divide this, assuming this was a geoboard, what's one way I could divide this up into halfs? Jonah? Jonah: You could take a rubber band and kind of strap it around like this. Jody: ALL the way down [points across the geoboard drawing]. Jonah: There aren't enough dots. Jody: Yeah there is. She's made them on the sides. KV: I see, Jonah was saying there wasn't another dot at the end. I drew a square around the dots SO it looked like it was closed in square. That's a good observation, Jody. When Jody said there were enough dots, Jonah extended his line across the overhead board. Jonah assumed this geoboard had 9 pegs and drew his line accordingly. He appeared to know how to create one-half with a horizontal line. Similar to the example above about Joanne's tenth triangle, Jonah's action seemed to indicate his agreement with Jody. Figure 4: Hannah’s Representation for a Fraction on May 3 The disagreement whose purpose was to mediate occurred on May 3 over a circle representation introduced by Hannah. Under discussion was an overhead question that Mrs. Vandenberg spontaneously created to conclude an episode of disagreement (the episode is discussed in detail in the next chapter). At the overhead projector, Mrs. Vandenberg pulled together different ideas on the floor to re-create Hannah’s circle and color in the two sections on the left, as Hannah did. Then she instructed students to write it in their journals and respond to the following questions: Is this 1/2, 2/4, or both? Why? When whole-group discussion resumed and Austin said, "It’s both," Carmen disagreed to say it was one-half. Although her explanation was virtually inaudible, we do know that she disagreed and that she thought it was one-half. As though wishing to alleviate a perceived distance between their answers and potential social distance between his classmates, Jonah appeared to mediate the disagreement when he called out, "I think they’re both the same but they’re written a different way." With this move, he linked two different ideas recently placed on the floor by two classmates. These disagreements to revise or correct - although focused on mathematical representations - pointed out minor glitches rather than getting at deeper issues Of sense-making. These disagreements converged thinking over a technicality. At first glance, they give the impression that students were mutually creating representations. However to correct another about a line not quite across the geoboard (as on Jonah's geoboard), although important, was not he same as disagreeing to defend an idea, as in the next category. Defend On the surface disagreements to defend an idea about fractions frequently appeared to be more adversarial than those focused on correcting, revising, and mediating. Actually, they served a useful function: to sustain two ideas on the floor, side by side, for scrutiny. Although prefaced in several instances by ”yeah but” statements, these disagreements did not place students’ mathematical ideas in polar opposition. Instead, they showed them to be willing to negotiate the floor for a turn and an idea equally important to them. Several representations generated disagreement which held up two ideas at once, including Rob’s geoboard. Rob's Way to Shpw 112 and 214 Figure 5: Rob's Geoboard Of 1/ 2 and 2/ 4 on May 7 One disagreement to defend occurred on May 7. The overhead questions during class were: Finding ways to show 1/ 2 and 2 / 4 on the geoboard. Record it on dot paper. Prove one of your ways. This was the third day of using geoboards, and on each day Mrs. Vandenberg asked at least one question about making one-half. But this was the first time since they began working with geoboards that she asked them to think about 85 —half and two-fourths at the same time. After the children worked in small ups and wrote on dot paper and in journals, Mrs. Vandenberg resumed )le-group discussion. By this time it was late in the lesson, and when she (ed for someone they had not heard from in a while she called on Rob. Rob's mesentation of 1 / 2 and 2 / 4 generated three brief disagreements, the first, own below, about the size of two squares adjacent to his Off-center diagonal. ee Figure 5 for Rob's geoboard of 1/ 2 and 2 / 4) Rob began to explain his way to nake 1 / 2 and 2 / 4 by counting pegs on the 2 / 4 side, saying he had 4 above and below the piece in the middle; and half on the other side (left of the diagonal). When Mrs. Vandenberg asked him what the piece in the middle was, Rob said he didn't know, "I just put it there so it'd be 2 / 4 and a half. ” When she asked the class what they thought Of Rob's way, one boy said he thought it looked equal. Investigating further, Mrs. Vandenberg asked Rob what he meant, ”They looked equal." His answer: ”because it's equal things.” Perhaps to learn more about their understanding Of ”equal,” Mrs. Vandenberg asked if both halves or sides were equal, and asked Moira to count the squares. Rob took away the band that made the parallelogram9, and Moira counted and said "They're even. They both have six.” Mrs. Vandenberg asked individual children, who said they counted seven and eight and that they have to put together half squares, too. When she asked Moira to check Off each square as she counted it, Moira marked them and said, ”Six and a half. " Shawna said it would be six if whole squares were counted, ”But if you take this one and that one (two partial squares to the left of the off-center diagonal) that makes a WHOLE.” 9This off-center diagonal as a single cut on the geoboard is an example of one-half, suggested by the authors of gaing Fragipng (p. 30) for teachers to have students prove. The 86 Figure 6: Halves Referred to by Shawna on May 7 A few classmates thought otherwise: KV: SO what do we CALL that? sts: It would be MORE than a whole. 8! Shawna: It's a little off but it's (gestures to the partial squares) still a half. Joanne: A little MORE than a half. KV: Is the other side a little more too? Mathematically, these children appeared to have different assumptions about the precision required to make one-half of a square, which was directly affected by Rob's Off-center diagonal. To Shawna's way of thinking, apparently half squares that were ”a little Off," when combined, still constituted a whole square. Proportion, while relevant, did not have to be exact. However, to disagreeing students, a half had to be a half. Rob's diagonal cut the squares so they were more than a half, and together made more that a whole. Based on Shawna's previous Observation that "that one and that one” put together would be one whole, Mrs. Vandenberg's question, ”What do we call that?” seemed to be about the whole Shawna constructed. A few students appeared to disagree over what the partial squares constituted, saying, "It would example in fling Fragtipns cuts in the opposite direction from Rob’s. 87 be more than a whole!" Their use of ”it" created confusion. The children appeared to use the pronoun, it, as a referent for different things. Whereas these disagreeing students used ”it” in a plural sense to refer to the reconstituted parts, Shawna pointed to the partial squares individually and referred to each as ”it." ”It's a little Off but it's still a half.” Also, it is interesting that earlier Shawn referred to these shapes as ”this one and that one,” but here she labeled one as "half,” which Joanne disagreed with (when she said "A little MORE than a half. "). Rob’s Off-center resulted in confusion for these children about what constituted a square and half a square. Figure 7: Moira Continues to Outline ”Squares” on May 7 If the children had realized they could examine these partial squares in a different way, they may have seen a pattern and, therefore, a connection, between the halves on Rob's board. This connection could have in turn led them to additional understanding about how to prove the left side was indeed a half (see Figure 8, below). Figure 8: A Way to Use Partial Squares to Prove Half. To encourage children's developing notions of proof, the authors of the fractions curriculum Mrs. Vandenberg used to teach geoboards offered suggestions to teachers. Specifically, when introducing halves with the geoboard, they suggested that teachers ”[Elncourage students to develop and articulate their strategies for proving their halves are equal and provide as many materials as you can to help students develop their own proofs” (Corwin, Russell, 8: Tierney, 1990, p. 16). Teachers were instructed to look for and discuss fractions which did and did not have the same shape and that they help children think about equal pieces. What about the sense Of proof during the disagreements over Rob's geoboard? What strategies emerged? Mrs. Vandenberg appeared to draw children' attention to proof by asking whether halves were equal. For example, when Rob showed his design with 1/ 2 and 2/ 4, she immediately asked, ”How is that 1/ 2 and 2 / 4?" Subsequently, she asked questions such as, How do you prove that that's a half over there? What do you guys think about Ray's way? Lots of you, almost ALL of you I saw found ways to show this. What do you think of RAY's way? This is equal to this? Let's take a look at just the halfs first. When Ray divided this in half, were these two sides equal? How can you tell? Do you think if we cut that out that would help us see the two halves? In addition, Mrs. Vandenberg frequently revoiced (O’Connor 8: Michaels, 1993) children' contributions during the disagreement. She echoed and amplified their ideas with these revoicings, saying or asking things like, 89 SO you found HALF on one side and you had 4 pegs and another 4 pegs. What do you mean, 'it looks equal'? They both have 6? Can you go point to them? Amy says you need to put together the two halfs. This was helpful. When Hannah was counting the squares, remember when she did this, so we could see? Regarding Mrs. Vandenberg's role, her initial question about Rob's representation brought out disagreement. While Moira outlined all partial squares along the diagonal, Mrs. Vandenberg’s second question, ”Is the other side a little more, too," drew the children' attention to the right side of the diagonal, to think about the partial squares there, too. When Moira was done, they all counted the squares on each side of the diagonal: ”Six and two halfs make seven.” This question was interesting because the children said "yes,” even though the squares pointed to on the other side were not the same size. Perhaps the children thought it all balanced out, basing their thinking on what they saw; or, maybe they didn't see a difference. Additional disagreement over Rob's geoboard representation was generally about the two pieces in the 2 / 4 on his board. Whereas, in the disagreement above, the children worked to figure out halves in the absence of Rob’s parallelogram, in the disagreements below, he recreated this unorthodox way of making 2 / 4 on the right side of his Off-center diagonal. In the first, described below, although he pointed to each of the fourths (in this case, the section Of the board above the parallelogram and the section below the parallelogram) on the board and counted squares, Ali disagreed, apparently thinking that the partial squares were thirds, not halves: 90 KV: SO Rob, how did you make it into FOURTI-IS? You've got your two halves [of the board] and we thought those were equal. Then what did you do? Rob: I took one rubber band, and made it go down. And then end up with 1, 2, half; 1, 2, half. 2 and a half EACH. Ali: 1, 2, and a THIRD KV: Ali's saying that's a THIRD? Ali: That's TWO pieces, not one. Rob: One right here, one right here (points to two parts) KV: One right HERE. OK. This is a way I saw lots of people talking about it. Here's a HALF (points to the left). Now I need to make 2/ 4 over here (points to the right of the diagonal). DO you agree? DO you think this is 2 / 4 over here, Rob? In the disagreement, Ali appeared to be saying that the pieces Rob pointed to were thirds of a square, not halves. And a look at the overhead geoboard on which Rob counted squares sheds light on this disagreement (see Figure 9, shown below). Figure 9: Overhead Geoboard Referred to by Rob to Indicate Fourths, May 7 When Rob counted squares in his ”fourths," he said, "one, two, half,” and pointed to the two empty squares and one partially empty square. To him, these segments Of the board constituted a fourth of the board. However, in his noting and counting of these segments, he omitted the partial squares outlined moments before by Moira. In other words, he did not take into account the segments of the board the children had just debated — the partial squares referred to by 91 Shawna and outlined by Moira. And Ali noticed his omission. She appeared to be telling him that he left out a part of the half square he had pointed to, to say, "'That's TWO pieces, not one.” Her meaning is not clear because the ”that" is not specific, although it is reasonable that she meant Rob counted only one partial square and there actually were two parts to that square. Rob seemed to assume that the partial pieces were halves, as indicated when he said, "One, two, half,” but was Ali talking about them, or something else? When Rob followed up and said, "One right here, he appeared to agree with Ali's Observation that there were two pieces. The issue was not resolved, because Mrs. Vandenberg repeated Rob's "one right here,” and moved on. What did these students need to think about to be involved in these disagreements? These brief disagreements originated from Rob's representation and had the students thinking about two issues. First, they needed to look closely at the squares on the geoboard and the way in which they were cut up by his off-center diagonal line. Second, they had to reflect on the question about making 1/ 2 and 2 / 4 on the board, and whether Rob's design did, in fact, do this. And, with a design like this which is not a cut across the diagonal, a careful look by these children was necessary. Their disagreements raised these relevant issues about squares and space on the geoboard. When Shawna said, ”It's still a half,” was she implying that because it's not a whole square it shouldn't be counted? After all, she went to the overhead and said, ”Six if you do that", then went on to talk about the partial squares making a whole. She didn't originally say the partial squares were halves - not until this speaking turn When Mrs. Vandenberg asked, ”Is the other side a little more too?”, she appeared to be asking if the partial squares on the other side Of the diagonal would be a little 92 more, too, because they were talking about those squares being half. Also, while Shawna and then Joanne spoke their turns, Moira connected kitty-corner partial squares on the other side of the diagonal. She did that while Mrs. Vandenberg asked the question about the other side. As they did in other disagreements, the children took up each other’s comments to initiate a turn to disagree. To get their differing ideas on the floor, they spoke for themselves to make the disagreeing moves. Because of the way in which they took a turn to disagree with a classmate in order to get their idea on the floor, this disagreement looked conversation-like and simultaneously challenged the children and Mrs. Vandenberg with new roles and responsibilities as discourse users. DiSaggeement Without a Visual Representation: Is 200 + 19 a Fraction? A second example from the category, Disagreement to Defend, came from May 3, the second day of fractions, when whole-group discussion was an extension of April 28. As described in Chapter 4, math time on May 3 followed Format 2, where the lesson began with Mrs. Vandenberg's orienting comments and whole-group discussion followed. After the children returned from lunch recess and settled into their chairs, math began. Mrs. Vandenberg planned to continue discussion from April 28 when she had the children work in small groups to respond to three exploratory questions, which she projected at the overhead: What is a fraction? When have you used fractions? How are fractions represented? Draw and label pictures. Whereas on April 28, the children worked in small groups recording their answers on large sheets Of paper for the majority Of class time, on May 3, Mrs. Vandenberg planned to begin with whole-group 93 discussion. Following one boy's comment about a fight earlier in the day, Mrs. Vandenberg immediately oriented the children to math time: KV: I'm gonna remind you right now there's no calling out. OK? If you choose to continue calling out you're gonna have to go down to the office. OK. Um, we haven't had math in a while on WEDNESDAY”, we started talking about FRACTIONS. And, THURSDAY was our field grip and FRIDAY we only had a half day. So we're gonna pick up where we left off there when we were talking about the three questions on the overhead. Jessica put your book away. Question Number One was 'What is a fraction?’ Question Number Two, 'When have you used them?‘ And Question Number Three, 'How are they represented?‘ On Wednesday we got to hear Bart and, Micah's idea. We also heard from, Tomika, and, ' T: Ali and Monica. T: Just a reminder. The REST of us, will be taking notes, keeping track of what DIFFERENT tables' ideas were. Remember how we started doing that on Wednesday? In a few turns at the start, she overviewed their previous lesson, the questions then and now, said they would be taking notes in their journals, then began whole-group discussion while focusing on the first question, "What is a fraction?” Mrs. Vandenberg first called on Monte, asking, ”What did you think a fraction was? How did you describe it?" He responded with words, and then wrote a number sentence and drew a picture at the overhead. ”A fraction is, 10All capital letters denote speaker emphasis. 94 where you have a big number and you can't solve it so you split it up into groups so it's easier to solve. . . A fraction is a piece of the problem", he said. While he talked he wrote for his example, ”200 + 10" and drew several of the following ”groups” (see Figure 10): Figure 10: Monte's Drawing of Groups on May 3. Others then aired their ideas when called on by Mrs. Vandenberg. For example, one girl wondered aloud, 200 + 10 is a fraction?", then said she thought ”division and fractions are related. . . Like you, you can make that [200 + 10] into a fraction. " Another girl, Hannah, who would be a prominent figure in the discussion that day, said to Mrs. Vandenberg that 200 + 10 was not a fraction. ”1 don't think it would be a fraction because 200 + 10, if it was a fraction, it would be um, less than like zero. You would like, you would take—you would have to take pieces out. . . So it's less than one." Apparently rnisspeaking, Hannah revised her statement during this turn. She first said a fraction would be less than zero, then less than one. She seemed to be saying that a fraction is part of one whole. Hannah's remaining comments on May 3 were consistent with this position. Subsequently, Hannah said, ”I'll show you what a fraction is." and went to the overhead to begin what would be an extensive explanation using 11Vertical dots indicate sections of transcript not in the text. 95 circular representations. First, Hannah drew a circle divided in half and said, ”There is a fraction. One-half.” When Hannah went on to say why 200 + 10 is not a fraction, Monte, still near the overhead, momentarily defended his position that it can be a fraction. They dueled verbally, point-counterpoint; 200+ 10 is not a fraction—it could be a fraction. Neither gave a reason for their position, nor did Mrs. Vandenberg seek one. KV: [200 + 10] Is NOT a fraction. But what you showed us is a fraction. Hannah: It's not a, it's NOT a fraction, because- Monte: But it CAN be a fraction. Hannah: because a FRACTION is less than ONE. TEN and TWO HUNDRED are not less than one. HERE is another kind of fraction. There are other kinds of fractions. THIS would be, one-fourth, if you shaded in one. KV: OK. Hannah: And, KV: How did you know it would be one-fourth? Mrs. Vandenberg repeated what Hannah said, then made a clarifying statement, ”But what you showed us is a fraction." When Hannah said 200 + 10 is not a fraction, Monte disagreed, to say it can be. Hannah continued her explanation, and Mrs. Vandenberg probed her thinking. This momentary disagreement demonstrated that these children were attempting to get at the core of the question on the floor, What is a fraction .7, and in the process of exploring the meaning of a fraction, brought to the fore two seemingly contradictory big ideas: that a fraction is a part of a whole, and that a fraction is part of a large number. 96 Kim’S Diamong A third disagreement to defend was Kim’s diamond. The cluster of single disagreements over this representation provided a window into students’ thinking about what they got when they combined two triangles, and whether the shaded portion on the diamond was one-twelfth or one-eighth. Figure 11: Kim's Diamond on May 11 This disagreement from May 12 raised the fractions notion of equivalence. This time the disagreements were prompted by a representation Kim initially created on the geoboard for a partnered task Mrs. Vandenberg prepared the previous day. Using a handout printed by Mrs. Vandenberg, on May 11 the children were to do the following on the top half of the paper: Draw a picture which represents your fraction. Explain in your journal. Then, on the bottom half of the paper, she wrote further instructions: What fraction does the picture above represent? Why? How did you solve this? In other words, the first child was to think up a fraction and draw a picture to represent it — without writing down or telling what the fraction was. (Also, the student wrote his or her name.) Then the second child was to name the fraction from the picture, and on the bottom half of the handout, write his or her name and explain how he or she solved it. On the handout, Kim wrote her name and on the back of the paper drew a diamond shape with one square colored in (see Figure 11, Kirn’s Diamond on May 11). Following Mrs. Vandenberg’s verbal instructions, Kim also drew the 97 picture in her journal and explained it. In her journal, Kim drew the diamond on the assignment sheet as well as in her journal. Under the journal drawing she wrote, "My fraction shows 1 / 8 because there is one colered [sic] in and there is 8 all together” (journal entry, May 11). Her partner, Jody, examined Kim's shape, signed her name, and wrote the following explanation on the handout, the owser [sic-answer] is 1/ 12 there is 12 things and 1 colored in so it is 1 / 8 1/12 it is a fraction because there is 12 things and 1 colord [sic] so it is a fraction” (handout, May 11). A bit difficult to decipher without punctuation and with misspelled words, Jody appeared to say that there were two answers to this fraction, one-twelfth and one-eighth, which she talked about when she was invited by Mrs. Vandenberg the next day to the overhead to share the shape she and Kim worked on. To begin math time on May 12, Mrs. Vandenberg said they would be sharing yesterday's pictures. Jody volunteered and went to the overhead with a prepared transparency on which she had drawn Kim's diamond and written, there are two ansers to this problem 1 / 12 1/ 8 Jody said, ”There were two ways I thought to solve this problem.” She counted 12 "things" said one was colored in, ”so I thought it would be one-twelfth. But when I showed Mrs. Vandenberg my answer she said that [pointed to the top corner of the diamond] equals one." Then, Jody counted pieces again, saying there were eight without counting with a one-to-one correspondence, number to piece counted. Subsequently, Mrs. Vandenberg observed that, ”A couple people 98 say they don't get it,” and asked Hannah to re-state what Jody said. In her own words below, Hannah answered, Jody thought at first there were 4 squares and 8 triangles and that's 12. And she thought it was 12 and 1 colored in, so it was one-twelfth. But then she said Mrs. Vandenberg showed her that each triangle, if you put 2 triangles together that would make one square. So, she counted 8, and 1 was colored in so it would be one-eighth. Elaborating what she said earlier about 2 triangles and one square, Jody above said when she put them together, what they equal was "just one-half.” Perhaps conscious of students' confusion about what Jody had been saying, Mrs. Vandenberg said, Jody, I think some people are having difficulty understanding what you meant by 'putting them together.' (goes to the overhead and writes questions above Kim's diamond) I want each of you in your journal, you might want to draw a picture to help you. But answer this. Pretend Jody was YOUR partner, or Kim, and you got this [the diamond] representation. What fraction does this picture represent? For the next 5 minutes, the children wrote in their journals while Mrs. Vandenberg moved from table to table, asking them questions about their responses. When she resumed whole-group discussion, several students said they wrote one-twelfth as the fraction in Kim's picture. When she asked Jody what she thought, Jody answered, Jody: One colored in of 12. And my other way was one-eighth. 1, 2, 3, 4, 5, 6, 7, 8. (points to and counts each corner as one) KV: What made you decide to think about it TT-IAT way? Jody: One of these (points to top corner) is a whole square, and those two make a HALF. As Mrs. Vandenberg worked toward gleaning Jody's thinking about her answers, Jody seemed to move from one-twelfth to one—eighth without firm convictions about either. Her dubious understanding of what fraction Kim's shape represented prompted several disagreements between her and Braden. The first disagreement between Braden and Jody raised the issue of whether two triangles in her rendition of Kim's drawing made a square. Braden said they were bigger, but Jody thought they still made a square: KV: One more time, Braden. Braden: If you put THESE two triangles together into one square they'd make a bigger square than one Of these, because if you put say just one of these triangles, take this triangle (draws a triangle beside Kim's diamond) Jody: Yeah but I thought it would still make a SQUARE so it didn't matter. Braden: Yeah if you put a triangle like this. (He draws one triangle) KV: I wonder if it would be helpful if we did this on the geoboard, if somebody did that one on the geoboard for us. How about Kim. Although Braden said putting two triangles from Jody's drawing together "would make a bigger square than one of these [a square]", to Jody it didn't seem to matter if what they made was bigger than a square in the diamond - two triangles would still make a square, presumably on or off the diamond. Then, when Braden appeared to start drawing his triangles, Mrs. Vandenberg suggested that putting the design on a geoboard might be helpful. However, while she was looking for a volunteer to use the geoboard, Jody tried unsuccessfully to put two triangles together to form a square (see Figure 12, Jody’s Attempt to Combine Triangles to Make a Square on May 12). 100 a Figure 12: Jody’s Attempt to Combine Triangles to Make a Square on May12 KV: How about Kim. Jody: THAT wouldn't make a square ANYWAY. Braden: Yes it would. Those two and those two. (he draws 2 right triangles so that they make a square) KV (to Braden): SO you're saying those two points in the square could be put together as a triangle? Whereas Braden formed a square from his triangles, Jody struggled to get it to work in order to support her claim that two triangles would make a square, so it didn't matter how big they were. At the end Of the above disagreement, Mrs. Vandenberg asked Braden a question to clarify what he said, but Braden either didn't hear her or ignored the question, talking next instead to Jody. Braden and Jody continued to debate how to make a square from two triangles. Braden (to Jody): But you see THAT would be bigger. Jody: You need something like this (again she draws 2 triangles, one under the other. I don't get it. Those don't look like SQUARE. Again, Jody attempted to create a square from two triangles, and again she faltered. Still, she held steadfast to the notion that they should make a square, and by doing so, continued to hold her idea up beside Braden’s. Disagreements to defend an idea functioned to sustain two ideas, side by side. In the moment they did not seem as collaborative as disagreements to correct, revise, or mediate. Neither did they seem competitive. The children 101 appeared to make this move to sustain a second idea, to be weighed by others on its merits. Thus far, I have discussed disagreements which functioned to pinpoint a flaw connected to a representation (Correct, Revise, Mediate) and disagreements which functioned to hold up two ideas simultaneously (Defend). The mathematical significance of the first category of purpose drew children’s attention to a surface characteristic about a representation. The mathematical significance of the second category was to get these children to look more deeply into competing ideas on the floor. The third category of disagreement discussed below, Question Another’s Assumptions or Call for Clarification, forced the fourth graders to look even more deeply at the mathematical content about which they disagreed. Call for Clarification, Question Another’s Assumptions This category functioned to get at the mathematical underpinnings of another’s position. For example, disagreement over Kim's diamond did not end when, as discussed above, Kim struggled to get two triangles to make a square. To the contrary, the disagreement continued and functioned to get at the core of Jody’ s reasoning about how much of the whole was represented by the shaded portion of Kim’s Diamond. Braden shifted the disagreement to be about the question on the floor: What does the shaded portion of Kim’s Diamond represent? TO demonstrate Braden’s topic shift, I pick up the disagreement where it left Off, above: Braden (to Jody): But you see THAT would be bigger. Jody: You need something like this (again she draws 2 triangles, one under the other. I don't get it. Those don't look like SQUARE. 102 Braden: When you wrote 1/ 12 down here, I disagree with that. Are you counting these triangles? Jody: 1/12. Ijust counted the triangles as ONE. Braden: like 1, 2, 3, 4, 5, 6, 7, 8, 9, 10, 11, 12? (counts all pieces, both triangles and squares) Jody: Yeah. Braden: Well that would only be 1 / 12 if they were ALL equal GROUPS. One of these (points to a triangle) is SMALLER than THAT (points to a square). It's half size. Jody: I know but THESE 2 together equals a half. But when I showed Mrs. Vandenberg she said why don't you just count THESE 2 together as one whole one. Braden: But you mean like these 2 (triangles) is one whole one? I get 1, 2, 3, 4, 5, 6, 7, 8. KV: Who can explain what the PURPOSE was for, how many people got 1/12? Raise your hand if you got 1/ 12 (5 sts raise hands) When you got 1/ 12 you counted this as 12 pieces? Abruptly, in the middle of disagreeing over how much area two triangles covered, Braden shifted the topic to disagree about what fraction of the diamond was colored in (the question Mrs. Vandenberg posed at the beginning Of class). When Braden said, ”THAT would be bigger,” he pointed to two triangles, maintaining his earlier stance that two triangles would be bigger than a square. Jody again attempted to make a square of two triangles, perhaps as Mrs. Vandenberg showed her the day before; however, for the second time she couldn't get it to work. When Braden shifted topics back to the fraction represented by Kim's diamond, he and Jody were quite specific about what they counted. When Braden replicated Jody's counting and came up with 12 pieces, Jody agreed that was how she counted. Then Braden raised a new point, that it could only be one-twelfth if all 12 pieces were equal. In addition, he appeared to change his mind about two triangles making more than a square, because he said 103 they were smaller than a square; a triangle was half size, meaning half of a square. At that point, Jody said that two triangles equaled a half, even though she simultaneously restated Mrs. Vandenberg's suggestion to "just count these two together as one whole one.” At the end of these disagreements which were prompted by Kim’s Diamond, Mrs. Vandenberg changed her question mid-sentence, seeking a vote on how many students got one-twelfth as the fraction on the diamond. Five children raised hands to vote, and several also said "yeah” in response to her final question, ”When you got one-twelfth you counted this as 12 pieces?" Perhaps because some still thought the shape represented one-twelfth, Mrs. Vandenberg showed Kim's diamond shape, which Kim re-created on a geoboard (versus a drawing). When asked, many children then said they could see that two triangles are the same size as one square, and that they thought the fraction that the diamond showed was one-eighth. In the end, Jody asked Kim, ”So what was your answer REALLY, Kim?”, to which Kim answered, ”one-eighth," and demonstrated how she added ”one-halfs" and wholes to get 8 total squares in her picture. Further, she added, her dad helped her, that ”he told her to write it down and add it. I tried it and he helped me. . . I knew how many halves to add because if you add 2 halves it would make a square.” Underscoring her thinking, she wrote in her journal on May 12, ”It repersents [sic] 1 / 8 becaus [sic] there is 8 squares 8: 1 is colered [sic] in so it's 1/8." Also, within her diamond drawing, Kim recorded the fraction each piece represented, 1 / 2 or 1, and wrote, referring to the triangles in the drawing, ”1 / 2 + 1 / 2 is 1 hole [sic] because all you have to do is draw a 104 N picture to make it" (journal entry, May 12). Mathematically, at the heart of these disagreements were Jody's false assumptions about Specific areas of this diamond. flat, she based her definition of a fraction on the number Of pieces in the shape without regard to their size. Her answer to the handout question, What fraction does the picture above represent?, was one-twelfth. In addition, she supported this answer with her written reasoning, ”the answer is 1 / 12 there is 12 things and 1 colored in so it is 1/ 12 it is a fraction because there is 12 things and 1 colord [sic]." However, her side conversation when Mrs. Vandenberg suggested putting two triangles together to count as one whole square prompted her to take this second idea (and different from her own about ”12 things” in the diamond) to whole-group. Jody shared Mrs. Vandenberg’s suggestion with the class (and Braden) when she put up the prepared transparency of Kim's drawing and her own thinking, ”there are two ansers [sic] to this problem," - 1/12 and 1 / 8. And, on the right edge of the handout she worked on with Kim she wrote, ”theres [sic] two was thig [sic] about it 1/ 12 or 1/8.” (That is, ”There's two ways to think about it: 1/ 12 or 1/8.”) In addition, in her journal she struggled with ways to put together two triangles to get a square, because there she drew a similar picture (this one of side-by-side triangles) to what she made at the overhead, above. Finally, not to be overlooked, Mrs. Vandenberg's suggestion, while well intended, may have done the opposite - either caused Jody to falter in her attempt to grasp how the two triangles could be combined, or kept her from looking closely enough at the 105 two answers she chose to be able to reason through which one made most sense. She did not appear to realize, either on the handout or during discussion, that to form a square the triangles had to be right triangles, and her repeated efforts to make it work failed. 25% she appeared to think that two triangles in her borrowed drawing made a square on or Off the picture, so the fact that they were bigger than a square, as Braden pointed out, seemed irrelevant to her. But if she maintained this stance, Braden's reason for disagreeing would completely escape her. Where he questioned whether two triangles of the shape would be the same size as one of the squares, Jody took the triangle-to-square comparison out of context. M Jody appeared confused overall about what, exactly, two triangles constituted. Her first position was that a triangle was one of 12 pieces, so should be counted as such, regardless of size. Then, to her it was of no apparent consequence whether a square made from them was a part of this particular shape or not. Presumably, her teacher told her that two put together made a square, so Jody interpreted that to be any square, on or off this shape. Finally, whether she misspoke or said what she meant, in her last speaking turn, above, she said two triangles were half, apparently half a square. In spite of the issues Braden raised and Mrs. Vandenberg's suggestion about combining triangles, Jody seemed not convinced that the colored area on Kim's diamond was one-eighth. A second important mathematical issue is the distinction these children needed to make between a fraction as a number Of discrete Objects versus a fraction as an area. Repeatedly, Jody worked at combining two triangles to make a square, and repeatedly her attempts faltered, even though her teacher suggested she do so. In addition, the shaded portion of Kim’s diamond forced 106 these fourth graders to shift their thinking back and forth, from what portion was shaded, to the relationship between that square and the diamond as a whole. A third important mathematical issue is relevant to these disagreements over Kim's diamond - the question of what constitutes the whole. Whereas under other circumstances, the geoboard may be used to represent the whole, in this instance it was not. Instead, it was used as a tool to create a different whole, the diamond. Therefore, a connection between what they did earlier in the fractions unit with the geoboard may have been lost. While the disagreements between Braden and Jody over Kim's diamond unfolded, none of the other children vied for the floor. While these two disagreed, what they disagreed about was quite clear. They frequently drew triangles and squares, pointed to them, and referred while pointing to "these” and ”that.” What was not clear to Jody was how to make a square out of two triangles. Apparently in an effort to help Jody "see” that, when combined, the two triangles in a corner of the diamond would make the equivalent of one of the diamond's squares, here, Jody either could not remember or did not know for herself how to put the triangles together. Repeatedly, she stacked one on the other and, when seeing they did not make the square she anticipated, she retracted her earlier assertion about putting them together. If she knew or could remember that in order for the combination to work, they had to be a particular kind of triangle (right), her confidence in what her teacher told her would not have been shaken. Additional disagreements arose which questioned assumptions held by others, including Hannah’s drawing of two—fourths to represent one-half. In 107 these disagreements, there were no disputes over the size of pieces, but how many, and why, as discussed below. Hannah' 2 4t how Half Figure 13: Hannah's Drawing of 1 / 2 on May 4 Here, leading up to the disagreement was a child's decision to create a drawing, rather than respond with a geoboard representation (which was what the question on the floor intended). On May 4 (the third day Of data collection and the second day they worked with geoboards), Mrs. Vandenberg wrote on the overhead and asked, How many ways can you show halves (1/2)? Children were to practice on geoboards, record their ways on dot paper, and write one explanation in their journal, explaining how they could prove their idea showed a half. Jonah showed his horizontal division Of the geoboard; then Mrs. Vandenberg asked Hannah to prove his way was a half. Rather than using a geoboard or the projected drawing of a geoboard to explain her proof, Hannah put up a new transparency to create a drawing (see Figure 13, Hannah’s Drawing of1/2 on May 4): KV: Hannah, how can you prove what Jonah showed us is a half? Hannah: Say you have a square...a brownie..And you have 4 people- yourself and 3 friends. SO you've gotta split it like this. Jody: That's more than a HALF. Jonah: Yeah. 108 KV: Why do you say that's more than one-half, Jody? Jody: Because it has four pieces. While she drew, Hannah appeared in the moment to create a story to accompany it. Her exploratory talk (Barnes, 1992) about the fraction and the story unfolded in the moment. She referred to her square as a brownie that she would split with friends. However, Jody, and then Jonah, quickly spoke up to express disagreement. Subsequently encouraging Jody to say more, Mrs. Vandenberg asked her about her idea. After Jody answered, ”Because it has four pieces”, Mrs. Vandenberg suggested they listen to Hannah, which they did. In her explanation, Hannah went on with her story, saying two of her friends went home, so they each got two, ”OK see how I colored that in (pointing to the half colored in) That's one half. . . One half. Four, take away two is two. Cause two plus two is four. It's a half. " Asking Hannah about how she proved her idea, Mrs. Vandenberg then asked, ”Are you saying that you could prove that was a half by seeing how much of the brownie you got? Anna, is that a way?” When Hannah answered, "Yes," Mrs. Vandenberg revoiced (O’Connor 8: Michaels, 1993) Hannah's proof, and then returned to the geoboard and the first question of the day, "Can you think of any other ways to show half on the geoboard?” This and other disagreements revealed an important aspect Of disagreement in this classroom. Through her question asking and willingness to have the children speak for themselves, Mrs. Vandenberg and the children mutually re-negotiated turns and ideas on the floor (Edelsky, 1993). She tended to ask questions which brought out more and more of the children' thinking. 109 And, whereas she Often was actively involved in the discussion through a teacher-student-teaCher-student turn-taking pattern, she also momentarily stepped back, so that children spoke up, student-tO-student and idea-to-idea, to disagree for themselves. Second (and characteristic of disagreement in this category), Hannah may have been more comfortable using her own drawing than the prepared geoboard picture on the overhead. Rather than responding to the overhead question with use of the geoboard as a representation of one-half, Hannah returned to a drawing to demonstrate her thinking. After all, on the previous two days of the fractions unit, it was drawings children used to flesh out ideas in response to the teacher's questions. Perhaps Hannah did this because she was less comfortable with the geoboard, or perhaps because at this beginning point in geoboard use, she preferred her own picture to explain her conception of one-half. Here, as she did before (and would do again), Hannah appeared to simultaneously create a story and a drawing, both of which used a different context and model from Jonah's geoboard half. Third, these children appeared to hold different notions of what constituted a half. Whereas Hannah first drew a horizontal line across her "brownie," similar to the way Jonah earlier divided a picture of a geoboard in half, she then added a second, vertical one, prompting disagreement from Jody and Jonah. Undergirding Hannah's representation seemed to be the assumption that one-half and two-fourths are equivalent. But to Jody and Jonah, Hannah created something more than a half, for they seemed to think in rigid terms: that one-half is one of two pieces. Also, they apparently compared the number Of pieces and saw the two pieces in two- fourths as more than the one piece on one-half. They seemed to be thinking in terms of number of pieces, not that the area on the right side of Hannah's square 110 was the same, whether one large piece or two equal-sized pieces. That is, Jody and Jonah may not have seen the equivalence that Hannah did. Also, they could have been tripped up by the way in which Hannah changed representations, from use of the geoboard (which the overhead question had them explore), to her hand-drawn square, or brownie. However, the question about equivalence raised here by Jody was explained when Hannah continued a story about having 3 friends, saying 2 Of them went home, so the two friends each had one-half of the brownie. Hannah had a clear sense of what the whole was in her square- brownie-and-friends scenario. While she spoke, she colored in the two squares on the right. Mrs. Vandenberg asked, "Are you saying that you could prove that was a half by seeing how much of the brownie you got? Hannah is that a way?” Hannah answered, ”OK, see how I colored that in? That's one-half.” Then, Mrs. Vandenberg redirected the focus and discussion back to the use of the geoboard to think about the overhead question, above. Hannah stirred further disagreement in the category, Question Another’ 5 Assumptions, with another drawing, this time of a television (”TV") to represent lfifi — _ _ _ — — ' \{ Figure 14: Hannah’s TV (Drawn from Side View) on May 10 one-half, discussed below. Hannah's drawing of one-half on May 10 was the object of two disagreements which functioned to get at the mathematical underpinnings of what can represent one-half. The premise of these disagreements was the 111 assumptions undergirding the positions of each participant. The overhead questions were from their homework: Where do you findfractions? How do you represent fractions: 1/2, 2/4, 7/14, 4/4? To discuss the first question, Mrs. Vandenberg asked, ”Hannah, did you find any fractions in your house?” When Hannah said she didn't have time to do her homework, Mrs. Vandenberg asked where she might look now: KV: Where might you look now, that you've heard some of our ideas? Hannah: At our TV. KV: At your TV. Why at your TV? Hannah: 'Cause I could divide my TV in half. st: NO. Tomika: How you gonna watch it? KV: Explain what you mean by that, Hannah. When Hannah proposed the notion of looking at her television to divide it in half, two students questioned it. Tomika's practical response went to the core of the usefulness of a television cut in half. Then, when Hannah drew a television and explained where she would cut it, children again disagreed. KV: OK how is that like a fraction? Hannah: You can cut it in half like this (draws a horizontal line through it) Tomika: But then it won't work. Hannah: It doesn't have to work... KV: SO you're telling us you can divide anything in half, Hannah? Again, Tomika's practical response, "But then it won't work," seemed to indicate that to her, whether or not a television would work was the issue, not 112 whether it could be used as an example Of the idea that anything could be divided in half, as Mrs. Vandenberg indicated. To Tomika, it appeared that what mattered was the functionality Of the television. Tomika’s perspective, the mathematics in Hannah’s drawing was irrelevant. There was no mathematics in it. Her concern seemed based on the function of the television. By contrast, Hannah appeared to have more Of a mathematical basis for her TV representation: anything can be cut in half to demonstrate a fraction. I included these examples of disagreement to demonstrate that hand- drawn pictures appeared to be comfortable representations for these fourth graders, more familiar to them than the geoboard, as I will later show. In the everyday world of children, there are brownies or pizzas or cakes to be cut. There's little doubt what the "whole” is, whether one food item or another. After all, these children created and frequently defined the whole. Not only did children have a sense of the whole in these situations, they had ways to ensure the pieces were equal. After all, in real life, ask one person to cut a cake knowing another gets to choose the first piece. In such an instance, children think about cutting equal pieces to be fair. But Jody's protest appeared to show that it is also reasonable from a child's perspective to question whether it's important to think about the size of a piece or the number of pieces. Also, hand-drawn representations demonstrated children' ownership of their ideas. In the drawings, we saw glimpses of how they used real life to connect to the less familiar domain Of fractions. Even one-half, considered to be the generic referent by children for a fraction, surfaced in the data again and again to be a complicated concept for these fourth graders. And in the case of Hannah's TV, we had a window into a real-world application of one-half (doubly 113 situated in Hannah's world by Mrs. Vandenberg, in her pre-disagreement question, ”Hannah, did you find any fractions in your house?"), taken to a level so abstract that Tomika could not grasp it. To the contrary, Tomika took Hannah's representation completely out of mathematics. Disaggeement Over the Location of 1(2 on the Number Ling Disagreement to question another’s assumptions also occurred over the number line, the most abstract representation used during the fractions unit. The location of one-half on the number line was especially problematic for Jody. A cluster of three disagreements erupted on May 13 when the question to begin class was, Where is IE on the number line? This was these fourth graders' first classroom experience with fractions on the numbers line, and it appeared to be a difficult concept for them. To Jody it appeared difficult, because three times she questioned others' placement of one-half on the number line. First, when Antonia put a mark mid-way between 0 and 1, and 0 and -1, Jody expressed concern that 1 / 2 could be in the domain of negative numbers: KV: What made you decide that? Antonia: I don't know. Or else it could be here (between 0 and -1) Jody: But that's a NEGATIVE number you're going by. KV: Antonia, why did you put it between zero and 1? Soon after, when Mrs. Vandenberg drew a new number line marked with numbers 0 to 4, She asked Micah where he would put 1 / 2. When he placed it between 2 and 3, Jody again raised her doubts: KV: Where would 1 / 2 be on THAT number line? Micah? Micah: (responds nonverbally by putting a mark between 2 and 3) 114 Jody: Idon't think so. KV: Why did you put it there, Micah? The third time Jody disagreed was when Joanne placed 1 /2 in the same places as Antonia had, between 0 and 1, and between 0 and -1. KV: . . Joanne, what do you think? Joanne: Here or maybe right here (she puts 2 marks; one is left of zero) Jody: I didn't think we were going to the NEGATIVE numbers! KV: What WOULD it be in between negative 1 and negative 2? Here, Jody again appeared to disagree with the idea that one-half might be with negative numbers. Jody initially seemed to Operate under the premise that one-half was a positive fraction and should therefore be with positive numbers. In this regard her assumption about 1 / 2 seemed mathematically sound, and mapped onto the first part of Antonia's and Joanne's assumptions. To Antonia and Joanne's way of thinking, it also appeared that 1/ 2 could be between zero and negative 1, perhaps because 1 / 2 was midway between zero and 1. In this way they appeared to assume that 1/ 2 could be either a positive or negative number. They seemed to sort of peek into the domain (and placement of 1/ 2) Of negative numbers. But Jody seemed almost adamant that 1/ 2 did not belong with negative numbers, and technically she was right. After all, Mrs. Vandenberg did not ask about locating negative one-half. Yet, it was not just the placement of 1 / 2 with negative numbers she objected to. She disagreed with Micah, also, who had put it between 2 and 3. When Mrs. Vandenberg asked where Jody thought it should be, she said, "I think it's between the 1 and the 2 because you've got the 1, and in between the 1 and the 2 ....you make a half. SO 115 you've got one HALF.” In her explanation, Jody seemed to be saying that one- half belonged between 1 and 2 because there is a 1 and a 2 in ”1/2.” This stance helps explain why she objected to putting one-half with negative numbers, but is less helpful when trying to understand why she objected to putting it between 2 and 3. She was comfortable having 1 / 2 between 1 and 2, which are whole numbers, but not between 2 and 3. The logical explanation appears to be that she thought this fraction should be between 1 and 2 because it contained these digits. Or, perhaps Jody thought that if the mark was placed between 2 and 3, that would be 2/ 3. The most abstract representation these children encountered, the number line as a conceptual tool to understand fractions appeared to raise more and different issues about fractions for these children. They were prompted to think more about one-half, but in a much different way. Not only did Jody ponder the notion Of a negative fraction it appeared that for the first time, she examined whether there was a relationship between how it is written and where it goes on a number line. As pointed out, above, Kim’s diamond prompted disagreements of more than one purpose. Kim’s diamond resulted in disagreements to defend and a disagreement to call for clarification. Monica’s drawing of three-fifths, discussed below, also prompted disagreements of more than one purpose. In Monica’s drawing, these disagreements were to correct (one to correct the discrepancy between what she drew and what she said and one to correct the representation) and disagreement to defend an idea. In turn, these different purposes functioned differently in the discourse, as I discuss below. 116 Moni ' uare: Five Pieces r F an Ar The E 1? Pieces of the whole in Monica's drawing of three-fifths emerged as the focus of three disagreements on May 10, the first two to correct and the third to defend. Her drawing grew out Of a discussion of her conjecture about 2/ 4 (see Figure 15, Monica’s Conjecture on May 10). May 10 was the same day Hannah drew a television early in the lesson and said she could cut it in half, to represent the fraction, 1 / 2. On that day after Mrs. Vandenberg got a listing of where the children found fractions at home, several gave examples of one-half (e.g., Hannah's television). Then Mrs. Vandenberg asked how they represented 1/ 2, then 2/ 4. After Moira and Connie described their drawing of 2 / 4 (a butterfly with 4 wings, two of which were colored in), she said she wanted Monica to share her conjecture about 2 / 4, which Monica did. "What I think the 2 means is how many are gone or colored in. And the 4 is how many there used to be.” While she spoke, Monica wrote and drew the following: Q --->how many are gone or colored in Q -->how many there used to be Figure 15: Monica's Conjecture on May 10 TO illustrate her conjecture, Monica used a blue pen to draw a square divided into four pieces. She colored in the right side with the same blue pen and said it was two—fourths. Mrs. Vandenberg then asked Monica about another fraction, 3/ 5. "If I just told you three-fifths, could you draw that?” Monica nodded yes, then modified her earlier square by adding an additional blue line: 117 Figure 16: Monica’s Drawing of 3/ 5 on May 10 However, perhaps because Monica wrote in this last dividing line in the same blue pen with which she'd shaded the right side (making the diagonal hard to see), some of the children seemed somewhat alarmed that she called it 3/ 5, and called out a correction: KV: How did you know that would be three-fifths? Monica: 'Cause the 3 is how many are colored in and the 5 is how many there used to be St: That's FOUR! Austin: That's two-FOURTHS. Monica: or how many pieces. Above, Austin and a second classmate momentarily disagreed about Monica's 3 / 5 drawing, calling out to her that she had four, and not five, pieces. This brief disagreement bubbled up, and the discussion continued. Mrs. Vandenberg asked the dissenters, ”Why are you guys saying that's two-fourths?” to which one student replied, ”It isn't.” Monica continued: Monica: Well, let me write it like this. (draws an identical but unshaded 3/5) Monte: There's 4 boxes. Austin: Not in the one up THERE! St: Five! KV: She did. You just can't see it. 118 After Monica drew the second 3/ 5 square, Austin appeared to change his mind about what she drew, saying, ”Not in the one up THERE!". He seemed to speak for her to correct Monte when Monte took the position that the drawing had four boxes. As with the disagreement just before, Mrs. Vandenberg prefaced this one with a question, and then bounded it with a follow-up comment. In addition to the above disagreements about the number of pieces or boxes in Monica's drawing, the children also raised the issue of whether the pieces should be equal. The discussion continued below, and the issue of equal pieces arose. Here, Joanne held a second idea up for all and seemed to say that the idea of 3/5 could be equal, but not the way Monica drew it: KV: I think that's the point they're trying to make. It's not equal. Joanne: THAT way it isn't. THAT way it is. KV: Do you agree with that, Monica? Monica: It's not. It's not equal. Joanne: YEAH it is. Just not that way. KV: Is it important that they BE equal? Mathematically, what were these disagreements about? In the first two examples, it seemed to be about 5, or 4, pieces, perhaps an issue of not seeing the fifth piece because Monica made and colored with the same blue pen. It was difficult to make the distinction between her original 2 / 4 drawing, and this one of 3/ 5, where she added a diagonal line through the box in the upper right corner. Maybe to the disagreeing students, the pieces simply were not visible. In the third disagreement, Joanne first seemed to say that the way Monica drew 3/ 5 119 created unequal pieces. But her second statement in the exchange above, "'IHAT way it is [equal]" raised a question in my mind. What was the "that” she referred to? The representations of 3/ 5 projected on the overhead screen were identical, except one was colored in (the adapted 2 / 4 square that was blue) and one was not. And, when Monica agreed that her pieces were not equal, Joanne used the unclear referent, it, for a second time. She may have held an image or idea in her mind of a way to make equal pieces of 3/5, but we do not see it in the data, for the discussion moved on. Here, she seemed to say that it is possible to represent 3 / 5 with equal pieces; it's just that Monica did not represent it with her drawing of a square. Discursively, the disagreements over Monica’s drawing functioned to pinpoint a flaw in the representation and to sustain two differing (yet unclear) ideas on the floor. The disagreement led to a brief discussion (brief because it was the end of math time) of number of pieces versus pieces of equal size. As alluded to earlier in this chapter, some representations may have been more familiar than others to these children. That is, when they moved from drawings to the geoboard, the sense of the whole apparently was not clear. The whole on the geoboard seemed not defined, either for or by them. And, whereas students’ drawings brought with them inherent issues of part-whole relationships (equivalence, equal pieces), geoboard representations raised their own set of problems, such as what constituted the whole, whether they should count pegs or squares, and on whose authority. After all, with drawings, the children themselves defined the whole by virtue of what they drew. Further, the number line as a representation seemed to deny these fourth graders familiar ground, territory they could create and tell stories about, either with pictures or, 120 to some extent, the geoboard. There were no parameters, no limits with the number line. And as discussed above, the location of one-half on the number line appeared to undermine the very sense Jody previously held of this fraction. When the representations became more and more abstract, these children had less steady ground on which to conceptually tread. Summary Where do these categories of purpose and function take us in our understanding of the mathematical and interactional aspects of disagreement? The functions of disagreement point back to the math content in them. In other words, disagreements which functioned to get at the underpinnings of another's position included those over the number line, Hannah’s representation Of 2 / 4 to show 1/ 2, and Hannah’s TV. These three representations forced the children to look most deeply at fractions content. likewise, disagreements which functioned to publicly hold up two ideas and which were prompted by representations such as Kim’s Diamond and Rob’s Geoboard, spurred the side- by-side examination of differing ideas. What did these children need to do to be involved in disagreement during math? As Mrs. Vandenberg and these fourth graders took on the more active role in discourse advocated in reform documents, what happened? Naturally, they needed to wrestle with big ideas about fractions, and demonstrate their academic competence (Mehan, 1979) which they appeared to do. Clearly, representations were prominent in the disagreements. Mrs. Vandenberg's questions and children' subsequent representations took participants into a variety of means by which to explore their mathematical thinking about 121 important fractions concepts, and included drawings, geoboards, and the number line. What makes a good representation from a fourth grader's perspective? These children drew on their experiences with circles, squares, diamonds, and even televisions, to incorporate drawings into their developing math understandings. For these fourth graders, representations seemed to be visual aids for students, almost as though they were a communicative supplement to words. And, as discussed in this chapter, some representations prompted disagreement. Some may argue that there's a lesson here, about the representations created by these fourth graders and described in this chapter. For example, Rob's design for 1 / 2 and 2 / 4 could be questioned as unusual. However, Mrs. Vandenberg's initial intent with geoboards was to draw out student ideas as they explored the board in conjunction with her overhead questions, as suggested by designers of the curriculum resource she used (California Board of Education, 1990). In the end, Rob's design forced such a close look at the board that the children had to think about issues such as whether counting pegs or squares made more sense, and what made up something as small as one square of the geoboard, even though a focus on what constituted the whole sometimes seemed to be lost. And to Hannah's way of thinking, a television in her home, when drawn and bisected, seemed to be an adequate representation of one-half. But these children learned more than math content. In addition to math, they were learning how to disagree, as evidenced by their ability to do so. This was a social competence (Mehan, 1979), an integral part of mathematical discourse. In the disagreements discussed in this chapter, the children most 122 often referred to math ideas, in part because Mrs. Vandenberg actively questioned them about their ideas and thinking. A few times when they used pronouns, such as ”it” and ”that," the referent was not stipulated or defined. For example, on May 7 when Ali claimed "That's TWO pieces, not one,” it was not clear what the ”that” was. We can speculate that she meant partial squares, because Rob again pointed them out, "One right here, one right here." However, most times, these fourth graders were specific about what they meant (i.e., Hannah's television, a negative number, four pieces, etc.). In the setting of fourth-grade mathematics where this novice teacher worked to foster the type of mathematical she envisioned to be aligned with current reform, she and the children bumped into strategic aspects of fractions learning. Part-whole relationships confronted them through their representations in most disagreements. Pictures and geoboard shapes led them to examine halves and fourths, equivalence, and equal pieces. And the location Of one-half on the number line perplexed Jody as she struggled with her understanding of one-half. In disagreement, the children’s world met the math world in two ways. They worked to learn how to disagree, and how be involved in mathematical discourse Of the type envisioned by the StandardS (1991). They tried on the language of mathematics, sometimes using their own words as they struggled for the conventional math words. In addition, they strove to appropriate the norms for participation in discussion and disagreement which Mrs. Vandenberg fostered. All in all, important elements emerged through these children' disagreement over fractions content. At times, Mrs. Vandenberg momentarily receded from the discussion so that the children negotiated their own turns to get 123 an idea on the floor to disagree. Teacher and students mutually negotiated the floor (Edelsky, 1993) to explore fractions concepts. The students received guidance from their teacher as they navigated their way through the domain of fractions, an area of mathematics considered complex by curriculum developers (Corwin, Russell, and Tierney, 1990). Her question asking and comments spiraled through their contributions to the discussion, as did children' contributions. During whole-group discussion, Mrs. Vandenberg appeared to support student contributions through questions and connections, nudging the discourse along. And, when children disagreed, the disagreements discussed in this chapter were usually framed by her comments. In addition, her involvement outside disagreement through frequent reminders about tasks and behavior appeared to shape the discourse so that the fourth graders focused during disagreement on math. However, as I discuss in the next chapter, this was not always the case. 124 CHAPTER SIX EPISODES OF DISAGREEMENT The previous chapter focused on the mathematical nature of single disagreement and demonstrated that these children were so involved in the discussion and with their ideas that they negotiated their turns and ideas onto the floor, resulting in disagreements over math content. This was not the case with episodes Of disagreement. Rather than the math, it was the interactions between the students which stood out in episodes, where they struggled with their social competence (Mehan, 1979). In the episodes, Mrs. Vandenberg inadvertently receded from the discourse, to answer the door or attend to a student at her side with a question. And, coincidentally, disagreement erupted at nearly the same moment she was detoured from her role as discourse conductor. But when she receded, the children had longer strings of talk as they acted to get their ideas on the floor. And when they did, they did not sustain the participation norms Mrs. Vandenberg worked to establish. However, the children were so involved in the disagreement that they went on without their teacher anyway, to the extent that Mrs. Vandenberg struggled to renegotiate the floor to manage the disagreement. In this chapter, I first describe the math lessons on May 3 and May 7, in which episodes of disagreement occurred. I then discuss the episodes, highlighting the mathematical and interactional aspects of each. And, I include a discussion of the mathematics task as well as the mathematical thinking revealed by these students, and the ways in which they talked and responded to each other. Second, I discuss what these disagreements seemed to be about; third, the ways in which these episodes affected the ensuing discussion; and finally, the 125 role of this teacher and these students in the disagreements. I follow this with a similar discussion of the episode of disagreement on May 7. I will show that when Mrs. Vandenberg receded from these discussions, the children continued, and the norms of participation she fostered took a back seat to norms the children may have experienced elsewhere. May 3 As described in Chapter 4, math time on May 3 followed Format 2, where the lesson began with Mrs. Vandenberg's orienting comments and whole-group discussion followed. After the children returned from lunch recess and settled into their chairs, the math lesson began. Mrs. Vandenberg planned to continue the discussion from April 28 when she had the children work in small groups to respond to three exploratory questions, which she projected at the overhead: What is a fraction? When have you used fractions? How are fractions represented? Draw and label pictures. Whereas on April 28, the children worked in small groups recording their answers on large sheets of paper for the majority of class time, on May 3, Mrs. Vandenberg planned to resume discussion where it left Off on April 28, by beginning with whole-group discussion. Mathematically, the discussion to come reflected these fourth graders' thinking about fractions, in part-whole relationships and as part of a set. Interactionally, the episode of disagreement revealed that there was more to disagreement over math concepts than the math. The world of these children outside of school, and perhaps their more natural ways of interacting, came to the fore as discussion of the questions of the day unfolded. While the children disagreed in the absence Of their teacher, the mathematics and norms of disagreement collided. 126 Math time began when, following one boy's comment about a fight earlier in the day, Mrs. Vandenberg immediately oriented the children to the task at hand: KV: I'm gonna remind you right now there's no calling out. OK? If you choose to continue calling out you're gonna have to go down to the Office. OK. Um, we haven't had math in a while on WEDNESDAY", we started talking about FRACTIONS. And, THURSDAY was our field trip and FRIDAY we only had a half day. SO we're gonna pick up where we left off there when we were talking about the three questions on the overhead. Jessica put your book away. Question Number One was 'What is a fraction?‘ Question Number Two, 'When have you used them?‘ And Question Number Three, 'How are they represented?‘ On Wednesday we got to hear Bart and, Micah's idea. We also heard from, Tomika, and, KV: Ali and Monica. KV: Just a reminder. The REST of us, will be taking notes, keeping track of what DIFFERENT tables' ideas were. Remember how we started doing that on Wednesday? Figure 17: Monte’s Drawing Of Groups on May 3 In a few turns at the start, she overviewed their previous lesson, the questions then and now, said they would be taking notes in their journals, then 1"All capital letters denote speaker emphasis. 127 began whole-group discussion while focusing on the first question, ”What is a fraction?” Mrs. Vandenberg first called on Monte, asking, "What did you think a fraction was? How did you describe it?” He responded with words, and then wrote a number sentence and drew a picture at the overhead. "A fraction is, where you have a big number and you can't solve it so you split it up into groups so it's easier to solve. . . A fraction is a piece of the problem,” he said. While he talked he wrote for his example, ”200 + 10" and drew several ”groups.” Others then aired their ideas when called on by Mrs. Vandenberg. For example, one girl wondered aloud, "200 + 10 is a fraction?", then said she thought ”division and fractions are related. . . Like you, you can make that [200 + 10] into a fraction. ” Another girl, Hannah, who would be a prominent figure in the discussion that day, said to Mrs. Vandenberg that 200 + 10 was not a fraction. ”I don't think it would be a fraction because 200 + 10, if it was a fraction, it would be um, less than like zero. You would like, you would take—you would have to take pieces out. . . SO it's less than one.” Apparently misspeaking, Hannah revised her statement during this turn. She first said a fraction would be less than zero, then less than one. She seemed to be saying that a fraction is part of one whole. Hannah's remaining comments on May 3 were consistent with this position. Subsequently, Hannah said, ”I'll show you what a fraction is." and went to the overhead to begin what would be an extensive explanation using circular representations. First, Hannah drew a circle divided in half and said, ”There is a fraction. One-half.” So, to this point in the lesson on May 3, the discussion about "What is a fraction?” included the idea that a fraction is a big number you split into groups, like 200 + 10 (Monte); division and fractions are related (Joanne); and that a 128 fraction (for example, one-half) is less than one (Hannah). As the children responded to Mrs. Vandenberg's questions, they seemed to be talking more to her than to each other. When Hannah went on to say why 200 + 10 is not a fraction, Monte, still near the overhead, momentarily defended his position that it can be a fraction (described in Chapter 5). They dueled verbally, point-counterpoint; 200 + 10 is not a fraction—it could be a fraction. Neither gave a reason for their position, nor did Mrs. Vandenberg seek one. Not responding to Monte, Hannah drew another circle, this time divided into fourths. Figure 18: Hannah's Representation for a Fraction on May 3 When Mrs. Vandenberg asked her how her circles were like 200 divided by 10, Hannah restated her idea and basically talked about 200 and 10 as whole numbers, then continued by shading in the fourths: Hannah: The reason, OK. Two hundred divided by 10 is not a fraction. . because. . a fraction is less than one. Ten and 200 are not less than one. Here is another kind of fraction (another circle drawn, divided in four parts). There are other kinds of fractions. This would be, one-fourth, if you shaded in one. KV: OK. Hannah: And, KV: How did you know that would be one-fourth. Hannah: It would be two-fourths if you shaded in, two, three-fourths and four-fourths. 129 Quietly, these fourth graders Observed Hannah while she drew the circle, this time divided into four sections. Speaking with more emphasis and apparent confidence (and volume) than when she first went to the front, her images of a fraction (the first, a circle divided in half; the second circle divided into fourths) were projected for all to see. Piece by piece, Hannah colored in and labeled each section, talking as she colored them. ”One-fourth. . . two-fourths. . . three- fourths. . . four-fourths, " she said. From across the room, another child, Ali, soon joined in the talk about fourths. As Hannah's explanation of a fraction unfolded, the others watched, wrote in their journals, and began to disagree. Ali reacted to Hannah's talk about fraction parts, saying, ”And one. Or you could say one," at the same time Hannah filled in the last piece and said, "Four-fourths.” Hannah: It would be TWO-fourths if you shaded in, two, THREE-fourths and FOUR-fourths. Ali: And ONE. Or you can say ONE. Hannah's "four-fourths” and Ali's ”one" hung in tandem in the air. Appearing to capitalize on an appropriate juncture in Hannah's turn, Ali put the idea on the floor that four-fourths could also be one. The discussion of fourths continued, first with a question from Mrs. Vandenberg: KV: How come you say one instead of four-fourths, Ali? Ali: ’Cause it’s, all colored in. It’s one, group. Ali, like Hannah, pursued the idea of a fraction as pieces that are colored in; however, Ali appeared to put the pieces together to think of the figure as a 130 whole. Mrs. Vandenberg then spoke to Hannah about the way she colored in her pieces. Hannah: It would be two-fourths if you shaded in, two, three-fourths and four-fourths. Ali: And one. Or you can say one. KV: How come you say one instead of four-fourths, Ali. Ali: 'Cause it's, all colored in it's one, group. KV: Hannah it was helpful to me the way you went through there and you colored a piece at a time, one-fourth, two-Did you guys see that? TWO-fourths, THREE-fourths. Hannah: In other words if you just put it in half, and you colored in half, it would be one, (She records '2 / 2' beside her circle.) Ali: Two. Again, Ali and Hannah's turns tumbled over each other, Hannah saying ”one;” Ali saying ”two” when Hannah paused in the midst of saying ”one-half" to describe the half she'd colored in. Rather than completing that last part of the fraction's name, Hannah paused, and Ali stepped in, apparently to finish Hannah's sentence, to become ”one-two.” For the second time, Ali conveyed her involvement in Hannah's explanation and disagreed, and at a juncture in Hannah's turn, gave her own interpretation of it. The first time Ali spoke of four- fourths as one. Here she said one-half would be "one-two,” and again spoke up at a juncture in Hannah's talk when she recorded, ”2/2”, as though this numeric writing of the whole finished her sentence. But not finishing this sentence allowed someone else to do so, which Ali did, as though she was thinking ”And when you color the other side, you get two, which makes would be one whole.” Again, Hannah did not take up Ali's call out, instead completing her own 131 thought, this time with words. As Mrs. Vandenberg stepped aside to talk to an adult at the door, in essence she receded from the interaction, which the children sustained: Hannah: One-HALF. It would be one-half. Ali: One-two is one-half. Jody: But you would STILL split up, two hundred and, Hannah: And you color the OTHER side would be Ali: ONE. Hannah: TWO, halves. Ali: OR you can just put ONE. Hannah: NO! Why would you put ONE? Taking the view that one-two is one-half, Ali appeared to be giving a second name to the same thing Hannah colored in (one-half); and seemed to be re-stating her previous position that when the whole is colored in (in this case, two halves), ”you can just put one." Ali drew on her own children language presumably to help support her developing understanding of the language of math, here taking her invention, ”one-two,” to link with the conventional, one- half. Whereas it seemed to me that the children were talking generally as a group, in part because Mrs. Vandenberg asked questions and connected students’ ideas, when Hannah called to Ali, ”NO! Why would you put one?”, the interaction became more focused between Hannah and Ali, erupting in a stronger disagreement over what to call this representation. Also, these students' interaction took a turn, evidenced by prosodic changes of voice. Ali's previous call outs were quieter, spoken in somewhat of a soft voice. But here, she turned 132 up the volume and left space between each word, ”All - filled - in.” They looked directly at each other, each taking up the other's comment. Whereas before Ali reacted to Hannah's comments resulting in two-turn disagreement, now the disagreement between these two girls became more entrenched, cycling through more turns examples. With emphasis and clarity, Ali responded to Hannah, explaining (a second time) why she would put one (versus two, for two halves): Ali: 'Cause it's L11 filled i_n_. KV: OK I missed that last ques- Hannah: NO! Whereas before Ali's contributions about one, and one-two as one-half did not appear to be acknowledged by Hannah, this "a_l fil_le_d i_n drew a reaction. Interrupting her teacher, Hannah declared ”NO!” to Ali's comment that she would put in one, apparently because the circle was all filled in. Mrs. Vandenberg continued, her question followed by Hannah's comments to Ali: KV: Hannah's last comment, was that that was FOUR-fourths. And All said it was ONE, because it was one whole-if we were thinking about that like a COOKIE, and you were getting FOUR-fourths, would you actually be getting one whole cookie? Hannah: Ali Ali I want you to think. This PROBABLY could be one- second, but then what would, THIS be? Ali and Sts: One-HALF. As she had done before, Hannah did not appear to acknowledge Mrs. Vandenberg's question, instead sticking with her line Of thinking and drawing more circular representations as she elaborated. More images emerged on the overhead screen, images that Hannah pointed to and emphasized again while 133 she simultaneously launched a series of question-answer exchanges. Also, Hannah's terms changed, and rather than appropriating Ali's "one-two” (for one- half), said ”one-second.” Interestingly, when Hannah interchanged these referents, she subsequently seemed to disagree with the class, here over the use of "one-half,” the term for the half-shaded circle she began with Her voice was loud, her words emphatic, and her comments direct. Hannah- One HALF. That's the same as one—SECOND. There's one-half, there's one-SECOND. WHAT is the difference between those two? Ali: Nothing. Shawna: Nothing she's just said that you can do it either way. KV: Oh so she's saying that it also would make sense for it to be called one-SECOND? At this point, Hannah's question-answer protocol began to take on the characteristics of interactional sequences described by Cazden (1986), found in most American classrooms: initiation-response-feedback (IRF), except it wasn't Mrs. Vandenberg but Hannah who took on the traditional teacher role to control the 3-part sequence, and the contents of the sequence appeared as: (I) Hannah: ...This PROBABLY could be one-second, but then what would THIS be? (R) Ali and Sts: One-HALF. (F) Hannah: One HALF. That's the same as one-SECOND. There's one-half, there's one-half. (I) WHAT is the difference between those two? (R) Ali: Nothing (R) Shawna: Nothing, she's just said that you can do it either way. 134 To complete the IRF sequence, Mrs. Vandenberg stepped in to say, KV: Oh so she's saying that it also would make sense for it to be called one-SECOND. . . In the exchange above, perhaps Hannah consolidated the ideas of one-two and one-second, used one-second, and launched into a position that one-half and one-second are the same, then asked about their difference. Possibly because it is what she proposed before, Ali is supported by Shawna, when she responded to Hannah, ”Nothing.” After Shawna explained what Ali meant, Mrs. Vandenberg finished the IRF sequence underway, revoicing the talk about one-second. KV: Oh so she's saying that it also would make sense for it to be called one-SECOND. . . Since we talked about one-THIRD or one-FOURTH? You're right. That is two ways to say it and and another way that we've gotten used to saying it is one-HALF. Mrs. Vandenberg seemed to acknowledge Hannah's shift from one-two to one-second, repeating them, then inserting the conventional term, one-half, to connect it with Hannah's use of one-second. Hannah continued with more description of one-second, one-fourth, and two-fourths, again addressing questions to Ali - and getting responses: Hannah: You COULD call it, one-SECOND. But the REAL one-second is THIS. Now, er- (I) Now, what would you call THIS? Right here? (R) Austin: I call it a HALF. (R) Ali: A quarter, or a fourth. KV: A quarter or a fourth? (F) Hannah: One-FOURTH KV: DO you agree with that Kim? 135 (I) Hannah: What would you call THAT? (R) Ali: Two quarters or TWO-fourths. (R) Austin: I call it a HALF. KV: Wait! I'm curious here. I heard two different ANSWERS. Um, ALI said it's two-fourths, and AUSTIN said HALF. More questions, from Hannah and responses from classmates started up further IRF sequences with Hannah at the helm. Without Mrs. Vandenberg to foster collaborative norms of participation, Hannah reverted to a school-related interactional sequence with which she was familiar: Initiation-Response- Feedback. Taking on a teacher role, a role different from Mrs. Vandenberg’s but familiar to many school children, Hannah asked questions which required one- word answers, and to which she gave immediate feedback. In spite Of Mrs. Vandenberg's attempts to re-negotiate the floor (and the disagreement), Hannah either did not hear or ignored her, and instead continued talking to Ali. While Hannah remained at the overhead with Monte nearby and Ali continued, a few students attempted to enter the fray. However, Hannah continued, below, while looking out toward Ali. Meanwhile, Mrs. Vandenberg attempted to point out two different answers she heard. But, again, Hannah did not take up her teacher’s comment, as more pieces were pointed at and another question asked by Hannah of Ali. Then, when Mrs. Vandenberg attempted to stop the discussion for journal writing, Hannah, then Ali, persisted. St: Same thing. Hannah: It IS the same thing. Now what would you call THAT? (points to filled-in 4 / 4) KV: I want you to stop for a minute and respond to that in your journal. One second. We'll come right back to this. Can I write down your idea? 136 Hannah: FOUR-fourths, not ONE-fourth. If it was ONE-fourth it would be one. KV: I want you to respond to this in your journal. Here's the picture Hannah drew you. OK she had divided this into four pieces, and then she colored, TWO. OK. And I want you to respond to this question: 'Is this 1/ 2, 2 / 4, or both? Why?’ Austin said one-half. Ali said-Hannah said it's TWO-FOURTHS. A couple of you said it's both. Tell me what YOU think and respond to why. And draw it-write the question in your journal please. Ali: But it has, FOUR pieces. KV: Does that question make sense? SO Ali you are starting to explain it NOW. You think because it has four pieces, you colored in two? You think it's both? Write the question please. Get started Rob. In these simultaneous conversations, Mrs. Vandenberg worked to get the children focused on a journal question about 1/ 2 and 2 / 4, while Hannah asked Ali another question, then answered it herself. What was Hannah saying? What was she thinking? The answer to these questions is not clear; however, at least two explanations are possible. First, she may have reacted to Ali’s earlier focus on "one” (meaning the whole circle). That is, early on when Hannah said, ”NO! Why would you put ON E?", she did not listen to Ali’s answer, ”Because it’s fl mag i_a.” If that were the case, Hannah may have thought at that time that Ali focused on one of the halves, not one whole. Therefore, in this most recent exchange, her statement about ”FOUR-fourths, not ONE-fourth...” (note the emphasis on one in one-fourth) meant that, from her perspective, Ali (earlier and now) dwelt on one piece, not one whole. Second, and more plausible given Hannah’s nature, is the possibility that she attempted to control the disagreement by turning or shifting its focus, a common discourse move among adults and frequently discussed in conversational analysis literature on male- 137 female communication13 (see, for example, West 8: Garcia, 1988). And, not to be overlooked is that when the disagreement erupted, Mrs. Vandenberg was at the door, essentially removed from the discussion for the moment. Although Mrs. Vandenberg fostered a different kind of discourse, Hannah may have reverted to discourse norms from previous grades. It is notable in these segments with Hannah that Mrs. Vandenberg worked to achieve different participation norms in the spirit of the kind of discourse advocated in reform documents. In these different norms where she could get ideas on the floor and children to reason about them collaboratively, it turned out that while she was the door, Hannah’s moves in the disagreement appeared to include two adult-like attempts to control the talk. She directed the disagreement through several IRF sequences and seemed to take tighter control by making a powerful discourse move: shifting topics mid-disagreement. To end the disagreement, Mrs. Vandenberg consolidated the final comments of children into an overhead question for their journals. When she did this she re-created Hannah’s representation of fourths and revoiced (O’Connor 8: Michaels, 1993) Austin’s observation, "I call it a half": KV: I want you to respond to this in your journal. Here's the picture Hannah drew you. OK she had divided this into four pieces, and then she colored, TWO. OK. And I want you to respond to this question: 'Is this 1/ 2, 2/ 4, or both? Why?’ Austin said one-half. Ali said-Hannah said it's TWO-FOURTHS. A couple of you said it's both. Tell me what YOU think and respond to why. And draw it-write the question in your journal please. '3 Interestingly, this literature focuses on the ways in which males control conversations (i.e., topic shift, interruptions). But in the instances with Hannah discussed in this study, Hannah makes these conversational moves (Tannen, 1990) frequently associated with males in literature on language and gender. 138 She concluded the disagreement by bringing everyone's attention to "different answers,” one-half and two-fourths, she heard and giving directions for journal writing. With Hannah's and Austin's ideas, she generated a question to which the class was to respond. Subsequently, students recorded and discussed the questions at their desks, then wrote responses in their journals. Although the disagreement ended, thinking about one-half and two-fourths did not. Following journal writing and discussion in small group at tables, whole group resumed. I discussed subsequent disagreement on May 3 in Chapter Five. To sumr‘narize, on May 3 Mrs. Vandenberg appeared to orchestrate discussion as well as the potential for disagreement. She brought more and more ideas and speakers to the floor, asked questions, and connected children’s ideas. However, when called to the door her involvement in the discussion inadvertently faded, and those norms she fostered did, too. The children reverted to their own ways of interacting, ways which may have been more familiar to them than the norms Mrs. Vandenberg orchestrated. But rather than deter these children, they did not hesitate in her absence, instead assuming the floor with their own turns and ideas, to the extent that when Mrs. Vandenberg attempted to re-negotiate her way to the floor, the children overrode her. And, in the case Of Hannah, some conversational moves (T annen, 1990) which controlled the disagreement emerged. By posing an open-ended math task to begin math time on May 3, Mrs. Vandenberg fostered a discourse space in which disagreement could happen. The discussion leading up to the disagreement seemed to position disagreement to occur. For example, Hannah's original thinking that 200 + 10 is not a fraction differed from Monte's. However, at the time Hannah was talking directly to Mrs. 139 Vandenberg, not Monte, so her thinking about Monte's idea went through the teacher. Ali's idea that you could call four-fourths, one, leaned into disagreement. Yet, when Mrs. Vandenberg asked Ali why she would say one instead Of four-fourths, the question and Ali's comments seemed to be between the two of them, teacher and student. The difference between the pre-episode airing of different ideas and the initiation of the disagreement was the moment when Ali spoke to Hannah and their interaction was directed to one another. Previously, with the exception of the somewhat isolated two-tum disagreement between Hannah and Monte ("200 + 10 is not a fraction,” "But it could be a fraction. ”) ideas, although different, were either addressed to Mrs. Vandenberg, or the children said them without uptake from others. What ia the Disaggeement Aboht? The math task on May 3 was discussion about three exploratory questions. The discussion of the question, ”What is a fraction?", stirred interesting mathematical thinking about part-whole relationships as well as the notion that a fraction could be a large number split into groups. Monte's notion that a fraction is a big number that is split into groups (e. g., 200 + 10) was described and discussed briefly; however, the majority of the discussion and its subsequent disagreement revealed the part-whole thinking of some of these fourth graders. What evolved as points of contention were what to call parts of a fraction. For example, before the episode, Hannah and Ali differed about four-fourths or one; two halves or one; then one-half or one-two. During the disagreement, the girls raised issues about whether to call the whole ”one" or ”two halves,” then, Hannah questioned the difference between ”one-secon ” or ”one-half." 140 Mathematically, the disagreements within the episode were about what to call Hannah's representations. First, Hannah and Ali disagreed over what to call colored-in portions of a circle (e. g., four-fourths versus one; one versus two halves). Then, Hannah argued with Ali and others that "one-second” (which Ali called "one-two") was "one-half” and seemed to quiz her classmates about the difference between the two. When Hannah turned Ali’s term for one-half ("one- two”) into ”one-second," Hannah argued that one-half was the same as one- second, which was Ali’s position. Whereas the episode was preceded by Hannah's explanation and drawing (and Ali's comments about ”one”) of what, to her, a fraction was ("less than one”), the disagreement seemed to zero in on three issues: a) whether to call two halves, one; b) whether to call one-half, one-two (subsequently changed by Hannah to become one-second); and c) the difference between one-second and one-half. In a roundabout way, Hannah ended up where she began, arguing about one-half. However, by this time, she controlled the debate not by logic, but through controlling the talk. Tha Interactions of the Children When these children disagreed with each other, they interacted with one another in interesting ways. They took it upon themselves to carry on disagreeing talk over a span Of time that involved several speaking turns, with and without their teacher. First, they spoke up for themselves to say publicly what they disagreed about. They also went about saying it in their own ways. This willingness to speak, disagree, and do so publicly seemed to indicate their involvement in the discourse and attachment to their ideas. Through tone of voice, use Of pronouns, and questions, Hannah, Ali, and others seemed to further evidence this attachment. For example, as discussed earlier, Hannah's tone of 141 voice changed to become louder and more emphatic. Ali also talked louder during disagreement, and when she said the circles were ”all m i_h", she separated these words to indicate the emphasis she gave them. m the children’s use of pronouns shifted, from a sense of collaboration to that of separateness. They initially seemed to use the pronoun, ”you,” in a plural or collective sense, as seen in the following pre-disagreement examples where the children are talking to Mrs. Vandenberg: Monte: A fraction is, where you have a big number and you can't solve it so you split it up into groups so it's easier to solve. Joanne: . . . division and fractions are related. . . Like you, you can make that [200 + 10] into a fraction Hannah: And you color the OTHER side would be Ali: OR you can just put ONE. Later, when Hannah initiated a disagreement with Ali, her use of "you” became more direct. When Hannah said, ”NO! Why would you put ONE?" she looked specifically at Ali, essentially intoning, "Why would you, Ali, put ONE?" With this move Hannah seemed to diminish Ali’s contention that ”one” could be a viable alternative. Also, Hannah appeared to separate Ali's math idea from her own. Her use of ”you” became more personal. Asking, "Why would you put ONE?” Hannah subsequently talked directly to Ali while also referring to Ali's idea: Hannah: Ali Ali I want you to think. This PROBABLY could be one- second, but then what would, THIS be. Hannah: One HALF. That's the same as one-SECOND. There's one-half, there's one-SECOND. WHAT is the difference between those two? Hannah: You COULD call it, one-SECOND. But the REAL one-second is THIS. Now, er- Now, what would you call THIS? Right here? 142 Hannah: What would you call THAT? Hannah seemed to single out Ali when she used "you” in her comments and questions, as though to lesson the status of her contributions. ma, when Hannah stood at the overhead and questioned Ali, at first glance she appeared to be getting Ali to clarify her reasons for calling these fractions what she did. However, the emphasis on the words in the moment seemed to me to serve a different purpose: to perhaps show that Ali was wrong. The children used other pronouns as well, such as ”I” and ”we." For example, Hannah said about 200 + 10, "I don't think it would be a fraction,” and ”I'll show you.” Austin twice called out in response to a question from Hannah, ”1 call it a half. " In these instances and others, when they used pronouns, they still appeared to talk about math; Hannah about the notion of 200 + 10 as a fraction and Austin about a particular circular representation of Hannah's. On May 3, the children’s uses of pronouns, as they appeared in the transcript, can be listed as follows: you can't solve it (Monte) You can make that into a fraction (Joanne) I don't think it would be a fraction (Hannah) You would have to take pieces out (Hannah) I'll show you (Hannah) if you shaded one in (Hannah) if you shaded in (Hannah) Or you could say 'one.‘ (Ali) PQNF‘WPS’N!‘ if you just cut it in half (Hannah) 10. if you colored in half (Hannah) 143 11. But you would still split up (Jody) 12. And you color the other side (Hannah) 13. Or you can just put one (Ali) 14. Why would you put one (Hannah) 15. Ali I want you to think (Hannah) 16. she's just said (Shawna) 17. you can do it (Shawna) 18. You could call it (Hannah) 19. what would you call (Hannah) 20. I call it (Austin) 21. What would you call (Hannah) 22. I call it (Austin) 23. What would you call (Hannah) Not until #14 in this list did pronoun use seem to shift from a collective sense to a sense I and you. In addition, their use of negatives was rare, as evidenced in the first instance from Monte, and the third from Hannah. A fourth way students showed belief in or attachment to their ideas concerns the use Of disagreement to appear to be right. At the time Hannah sort of twisted Ali's ”one-two” and argued that one-second and one-half are the same, this argument seemed Odd to me. I wondered, ”Isn't she arguing with students although they actually agree with her?” Analyses showed that they did, indeed, agree with her. Hannah's argumentative stance perplexed me. She seemed to be jockeying for the position of ”the one who is right,” and therefore, has to make it appear as if others are wrong. A good way to accomplish this is to make it look as if they are disagreeing with her, or that she is disagreeing with them. 144 The fact that the children made connections to each other's referents indicated their involvement and interest in this discussion and subsequent disagreement. In large part, their referents were clear. For example, early in the discussion, Monte said, "A fraction is where you have a big number and you can't solve it so you split it up into groups so it's easier to SOLVE." Here, he responded to the first question of the day which Mrs. Vandenberg asked him about, ”What is a fraction?” Soon, Joanne too, connected her thinking to Monte's when she said, ”You can make that [referring to Monte's number sentence, 200 + 10] into a fraction. " This pattern continued before and during the disagreement. To begin the disagreement with Ali, Hannah used both a negative and a singular (Ali)” you” to say, ”NO! Why would you put ONE?” She spoke directly to Ali and she specified the math-related aspect to which Ali referred (i.e., coloring in two halves is the same as one whole). And soon after, when Hannah said to Ali, ”1 want you to think,” she again referred specifically to Ali and continued to talk about a specific referent, her drawn circle, the object of what she wanted Ali to think about. In other exchanges during the disagreement, Hannah said, ”What would you call THIS?" and ”You COULD call it. . .”, and again, she referred to something specific when she pointed to her circles, projected from the overhead. In essence, she asked Ali what she would call the drawn representation, then said what this circle could be called. These child connections of the specific referents seemed to mirror Mrs. Vandenberg's weaving Of ideas into the lesson and further evidenced their belief in an attachment to their ideas. However, more prominent is how Hannah used language to control the floor in a competitive way much different from the collaborative floor Mrs. Vandenberg 145 fostered. Hannah got louder and used pronouns which separated classmates and their ideas. She also questioned Ali in a face-off of ideas, ironic because she negotiated the floor by getting a turn and initially contributing an idea (e.g., a fraction is less than one) to the discussion of the overhead question, What is a fraction? But she fell short Of succeeding because she did not know how to hold the floor. Her controlling actions backfired when she ended up essentially disagreeing with herself. Without Mrs. Vandenberg to manage the disagreement, Hannah’s own way of interacting interfered with the math content she attempted to get on the floor. The Teacher’s Rolg Mrs. Vandenberg took on strategic responsibilities in her role as discourse conductor: she seemed to allow the children to speak for themselves; she connected students' mathematical ideas across the lesson, and; she attempted to fuel disagreement. Early in the May 3 math lesson, Mrs. Vandenberg bore the responsibility for orchestrating the discourse. However, once disagreement was underway, the children came to the fore, as if to guide it themselves. Whereas Mrs. Vandenberg called on individual children, sought and connected their mathematical ideas, and asked questions, during disagreement the children frequently spoke for themselves, sometimes appearing to either not hear or disregard Mrs. Vandenberg. Second, integral to the forward momentum of this discussion and disagreement was Mrs. Vandenberg’s teacher role as discourse conductor through which she connected children’s math ideas. For example, in her orienting comments, Mrs. Vandenberg spoke initially at a general level, 146 connecting the May 3 lesson with the previous one on April 28 when she said, "we're gonna pick up where we left Off there when we were talking about the three questions on the overhead." She then talked about the three focal questions and said which children shared their ideas. As new thinking emerged, she then connected specific comments. For example, when Monte first proposed that a fraction is a big number you split up and created his picture of "groups," she said, "OK so you're dividing 200 by 10 so you're making groups of ten. How many groups would you make total?" Their interaction continued: KV: A fraction is a number, that you split up? IS THAT how you explained it?. Monte: Yup. What you can do, to make this problem easier is you can split it up into groups of 10. 1, 2, 3, 4, 5, 6, 7, 8, 9, 10. There's one group. . . KV: OK so you're dividing 200 by 10 so you're making groups of ten. How many groups would you make total? Monte: Twenty. KV: Twenty groups. And how is that, related to fractions. Monte: Well if you're splitting a number up into groups to, tO make it easier. KV: How is that DIFFERENT than dividing. KV: You showed us a DIVISION problem 200 + 10, and then you said you'd go on to make 20 groups of 10? Monte: Yeah. KV: And you're saying that's a fraction? 147 Monte: Yeah 'cause a fraction is a PIECE of the problem. KV: A PIECE of the problem. What do you guys think about that. Monte responded to the question, 'What is a fraction?', by writing this DIVISION problem. Any responses? KV: Comments, questions for him? Mrs. Vandenberg also asked and then connected Braden's and Joanne's ideas about Monte's idea that 200 + 10 is a fraction: KV: What do you think about MONTE'S idea? Braden: From what I HEARD— KV: DO you AGREE with it or— Braden: Well—what I HEARD I don't really think so. KV: Would you tell him again, Braden. Braden: I RECORDED the first part of what he said. KV: HE showed us a division problem, 200 + 10, and he said THAT would be a fraction. Braden: I don' agree with him because um, KV: You AGREE with him? Braden: NO I DON'T agree with him. KV: You DON'T agree with him. Braden: Because a HUNDRED divided by 10, would be 10. KV: OK. Joanne: Two hundred divided by 10 would be a fraction? KV: Braden says he doesn't agree. Joanne you're saying you AGREE. 148 Joanne: In a WAY. KV: Why 'in a way.’ As the dialogue continued, Mrs. Vandenberg continued to connect each child's idea with what was said before, including the next to speak: Hannah. KV: Hannah? Hannah: I might be able to solve 200 + 10. . . KV: You think with some color tiles you could solve that problem. Is that a fraction? Hannah: I don't think it would be a fraction because 200 + 10, if it was a fraction—er not a fraction—if it was a fraction,. . it would be um, less than like ZERO you would like, you would take—you would have to take pieces OUT. KV: Hannah you're saying— Hannah: So it's less than ONE. like THAT‘S what a fraction—here I'll show you what a KV: You're saying a fraction is less than ZERO? After more comments, Mrs. Vandenberg connected Hannah's ideas with Ali's: Hannah: HERE is a fraction. THERE is a fraction. ONE-HALF. KV: OK. That reminds me of what ALI showed us. Do you remember how ALI showed us her way of dividing up circles? Joanne: Yes see, KV: And how is THAT like 20 + 10 Hannah. Or TWO HUNDRED divided by 10 excuse me. Hannah: I keep making my two sideways. OK The REASON, OK. Two hundred divided by 10 is not a fraction. KV: Is NOT a fraction. But what YOU showed us IS a fraction. 149 The third way in which Mrs. Vandenberg evidenced her role as discourse conductor was her teacher moves which opened the door for disagreement. Whereas she sometimes succeeded in stirring disagreement, there were also times when the children did not take the bait. As discussed above, Mrs. Vandenberg often made connections across student ideas, and those linked ideas were not necessarily identical or similar. Also, after she invited a child to share his or her idea about the question or problem of the day and that child had done so, Mrs. Vandenberg would ask questions such as, "What do you think, Kim?", "Braden, what do you think about that?", ”What do you guys think?", ”Do you agree?", or ”Do you disagree?” These questions acted as stimulants for more students' ideas, thereby potentially getting more ideas on the floor. To summarize, this teacher's role in orchestrating the discussion and disagreement served to position disagreement in several ways. First, as in the disagreements discussed in Chapter Five, Mrs. Vandenberg frequently allowed the children to initiate their own turns to disagree with each other. Here, when she receded, the children also initiated their own speaking turns and continued the disagreement in her absence, to the point that she had difficulty negotiating her way back to the floor. Second, she frequently made connections across mathematical ideas from the children about the question, ”What is a fraction?" When she connected and revoiced (O’Connor 8: Michaels, 1993), she placed ideas on the floor for consideration by all. She sometimes identified particular children and their association with an idea. She reached back into previous dialogue and nudged discussion forward through these links. Third, Mrs. Vandenberg Opened the door for disagreement by asking different students’ questions about what they thought about others' ideas, and whether they agreed. Over time, as more 150 ideas surfaced, she seemed to Work the discussion toward consideration of the multiple ideas on the floor, so that a result on May 3 was a disagreement which involved several speakers and ideas, and lasted several minutes. Eventually, some of the children spoke up for themselves, locked onto other ideas, and disagreed. pr Does ThiS EpiSOde of Disaggeemant Affect the Di§cussion? On May 3, up to the point when Mrs. Vandenberg generated the overhead question in the midst Of the disagreement, the children aired several ideas. Before the disagreement when they responded to the beginning question, ”What is a fraction?”, the main issues raised by students were: Monte: A big number you split into groups, with 200 + 10 and circled groups of dots as his representation. Hannah: A fraction is less than one, with a circle divided first in half and then fourths as her representation. Ali: Four-fourths is the same as one, because it's one group. . Ali: One-two is the same as one-half, because it's all colored in. Ali: One is the same as two halves. Students assumed responsibility for sustaining discourse in disagreement. In addition, there is another way in which this episode of disagreement affected the discussion: it provided impetus for more discussion. In the midst of the May 3 episode of disagreement, Mrs. Vandenberg created a discourse-generated journal question at the overhead for the children to record and respond to. After writing, she resumed the discussion, which was a direct result of the disagreement. She developed an immediate connection to certain students' publicly-shared ideas: Hannah's about two-fourths, and Austin's about one-half. 151 In this instance with the journal writing, she seemed to sort of put the disagreement on a discursive burner, to simmer it and then use it to further the resulting discussion. She appeared to capitalize on the moment near the end of the disagreement when Hannah asked a question about a specific question: "What is the difference between those two [her representations of one-half and two-fourthsl?” The integrity of children's contributions during the disagreement was sustained by this teacher's spontaneously-created journal questions which she posed from the overhead. To summarize, Mrs. Vandenberg started May 3 with a general question, ”What is a fraction?” As a result of discussion, different ideas emerged, eventually resulting in an episode of disagreement. During the disagreement, Mrs. Vandenberg created, in flight, an overhead question directly connected to one asked by Hannah. When she did this, the discussion around the representations for one-half and two-fourths stopped, but children's thinking did not. The overhead question appeared to sustain thinking and fuel further discussion about two specific fractions. In other words, Mrs. Vandenberg initiated the discussion with an exploratory question, orchestrated discussion through which children talked about their different ideas, navigated the disagreement, and concluded it with an overhead question about specific, child- generated math ideas. In spite of Hannah's use of pronouns, question-asking, interchange of one-two to one-second, and argument about the difference between them, the math-related aspects of this episode seemed to prevail. Mrs. Vandenberg's mid-disagreement overhead question appeared to sustain the prevalent math ideas, which she then used to launch further discussion about two specific and disagreement-related fractions, one-half and two-fourths. 152 Generally, the disagreement on May 3 appeared to be generative, because it promoted further discussion about whether two fractions, one-half and two- fourths, were the same. The notion that these two fractions might be the same originated in the disagreement and were suggested by participants. And, discussion about them continued after the episode of disagreement. Different ideas emerged early on in the discussion, followed by Hannah's initiation of disagreement with Ali. Mrs. Vandenberg sustained the disagreement with the journal question, then nurtured further thinking about the specific fractions, one- half and two-fourths, by orchestrating more post-disagreement discussion. However, the children’s ideas after the episode were very similar, resulting in a discussion rather like a flat line, until the end of math time. May 7 Because the content of math time on May 7 is rooted in previous lessons, I begin with a brief review of what happened before: May 4, the first day that students used geoboards, and May 5, when they raised the notion of counting pegs and squares to find a half on the geoboard. On May 4 (Day 3 of my study) Mrs. Vandenberg introduced geoboards as another means to have students explore fractions. Drawing on the teaching resource, Sgeing Fractions (Corwin, Russell, and Tierney, 1990), her plan was for students to explore the space on the board. Therefore, as suggested in m Fractions she introduced a question about making halves. At the overhead she projected the questions for the day and told the children they were to work with both geoboards and dot paper (a sheet with 12 reproductions of a geoboard). On the boards, she said, they were to practice, and then record their ways on the 153 paper. They were to also write in their journals, and later would discuss their halves. On May 5 (Day 4), students again worked with geoboards to continue thinking about half Of the board. Whereas on May 4 the children generally worked with their boards, paper, and journals, on May 5, more class time was dedicated to whole-group discussion. At the overhead, Mrs. Vandenberg wrote the questions for the day: Sharing ways to make a half. From yesterday, with geoboards. Which are congruent? Which are equal? On this second day of geoboard use, the final two questions, ”Which are congruent? Which are equal?", were new to the children. Much Of the ensuing discussion honed in on using squares on the board to make half, although some children thought they should count pegs. Math on May 7 Friday, May 7 (Day 5 of this study), was significant because two simultaneous disagreements occurred (see Appendix E). What interested me again (as on May 3) was the math content, the ways in which students disagreed about it, and the fact that what stood out was students’ interactions rather than the math about which they disagreed. This episode of disagreement differed from any other during the study in its nature and effect on the discussion because Mrs. Vandenberg had to step in to stop the disagreement. When she did, she stated the norms for participation in disagreement: "the children were to disagree with ideas and not people; *the children were not to raise a hand to or yell at one another. 154 In this section, focusing on disagreements of May 7, I continue the format of first describing the lesson and its disagreement, again pointing out mathematical and interactional aspects during the disagreement. Next, I describe the focus of the disagreements, and the ways in which they affect the discussion. Finally, I describe the role of this teacher and the students in the disagreements. The questions and discussion that day were a carryover from the May 5 introduction of congruent and equal. After a brief comment about a rough recess, Mrs. Vandenberg began the lesson by telling the children that two girls, Michaela and Antonia, had ideas to share. First Michaela, then Antonia, both soft-spoken girls, displayed and described their boards showing halves. At several points when each girl spoke, Mrs. Vandenberg asked, ”Talk louder, please,” and said, ”I can't hear you.” Whereas their representations differed, both said they counted pegs (Michaela said ”nails.”) to make halves on their board. Figure 19: Michaela's Way to Make Half on May 7 Several times during Michaela's explanation, children raised the issue of equal pieces. For example, 155 Michaela: Then there's, twelve in there. And then here's another 12 and you've got one nail left over. So I took that" nail and I made up nails with rubber bands to make it equally. Tomika questioned Michaela's use Of extra bands to "make up nails,” and asked about putting bands on existing pegs (versus laying bands where there were no P985). T: Tomika doesn't understand or agree with the rubber bands in the middle part. Michaela: Well if there IS a nail you make it EQUALLY. SO I made a part with rubber bands. Tomika: But, but all these right HERE, I don't I think it, just, I think you're supposed to like make it equally, equally, ee-qual-ly. St: Equally! Tomika: Yeah. Equally with the pegs that are ON there. Tomika: And PLUS, the pegs that are right there and are the same as like, on the other geoboards. T: The difference here is that, my directions were to divide up the geoboard evenly. SO the way, MICHAELA divided it up evenly she found she needed to ADD pegs. Like Tomika, others continued to wonder about Michaela's use of the geoboard. While Michaela continued to explain, Mrs. Vandenberg stepped to the overhead, pointed to Michaela's representation, and asked, the group, ”IS THIS piece equal to THIS piece?” Some children called out, "Yes," but Tomika noted, ”But there's one more LEFT!” Mrs. Vandenberg noted their shape and said, 14I continue to use all capitals to indicate a speaker's emphasis on particular words. 156 T: They're even the same SHAPE. What do we call it when they're the same SHAPE? Sts: Congruent! T: Congruent. Good She used Michaela's shapes to mention congruence and seemed to begin a transition for the next girl to go to the overhead and talk about her way to make halves. Whereas Michaela talked about counting pegs as the criteria for figuring out if she made halves on the board, others, like Tomika, Joanne, and Jody raised questions about the lone peg in the middle of the board left out Of Michaela's shoe shapes. Noting this, Mrs. Vandenberg said, "She made TWO equal pieces, which was part of our direction. Divide your geoboard into half or into two equal pieces. But she did it in a way that left this out. So she was trying to find a way to, to justify that, right?" Throughout, Mrs. Vandenberg asked Michaela about her board, touching on issues of counting pegs, and notions raised in the questions of the day: congruence and equal. Saying she thought Michaela's "was a different way to think about the board," she then invited Antonia to share her idea. Antonia, like Michaela, talked about counting pegs, saying ”Miss Schram15 and me were talking about Ali's idea'6.” When she paused to recreate Ali's idea on the overhead geoboard, Mrs. Vandenberg called on Braden, soon to be a participant in a disagreement. He went to the overhead, and to demonstrate his way, started to rearrange Antonia's board. Braden stumbled a bit in his explanation, beginning and stopping, looking confused at the shape he created 15Antonia referred to Pamela Schram, the Mathematics Study Group Project's Principal Investigator, who weekly visited this classroom. 157 and telling Mrs. Vandenberg, ”That's not what I did.” He counted the pegs to the left and right of the jagged line on the overhead geoboard. Appearing to note his confusion, Mrs. Vandenberg suggested he show the class the geoboard he was working on, which he did. Braden's shape looked like Antonia's. Figure 20: Antonia's and Braden's Ways to Make Half on May 7 While Braden held his geoboard up to the class, Joanne apparently disagreed with his method of proving half: counting pegs: Joanne: Wait a minute though! That’ s counting the pegs not the squares. Suzanne: But they’re not the same size. Moira: Yeah if you put that ONE thing that’ 5 going like a (in the air with her finger, she makes a horizontal line) When Joanne called out her disagreement for the first time in this lesson the issue of counting squares as a method to figure out halves came up. Discussed at length on May 5, I was struck by the fact that it came to the floor this late on May 7. Soon the discourse branched Off into two heated disagreements, which occurred at the same time Mrs. Vandenberg receded from the discussion to talk privately with Shawna. The first disagreement, near the 1""Ali's idea" refers to a previous discussion where Ali created a shape like Antonia's (above), recorded it on dot paper, cut it out, and said the parts of the shape were not congruent. 158 camera and between Tomika and Braden, was about whether Braden said to not count the pegs. Tomika: But you said up THERE (pointing to the overhead where Braden had been standing) not counting the pegs Braden: I mean not counting the pegs in the middle. Tomika: But you didn’t SAY in the middle. You're talking about right THERE. You’re talking about right THERE in the middle. Tomika initiated disagreement with Braden and emphasized her resistance to an earlier statement of his, that said he would not count the pegs. To clarify, he said, ”I mean not counting the pegs in the middle.” However, Tomika persisted, and got very specific about what he did and did not say. ”But you didn't SAY in the middle. You're talking about right THERE. You're talking about right THERE in the middle." I heard a definite undertone of noise while many students called out overlapping comments, adding to my confusion about what was happening to the discussion about counting pegs on geoboards. Meanwhile, a second disagreement brewed across the room. Like water about to boil, Joanne’s voice began to rise and Hannah’s animated movement stood out. The two girls, sitting on the far side of the room close to where Mrs. Vandenberg stood, appeared to be talking. I recognized Joanne's voice and saw that Hannah talked, too, but could not tell what they were saying. Joanne said something about ”pegs” and ”equal,” but in spite of my best efforts, I could not figure out any more. Hannah responded, leaning in toward Joanne, who sat kiddy-corner across the cluster of desks. I was about to learn through the children’s talk of their ideas about whether they should count pegs or squares to come up with half of the board, 159 and how vested in their ideas some children could be. Whereas earlier the first girls, Michaela and Antonia, touched mostly on the notion of counting pegs, eventually the idea of counting squares on the board emerged as integral to the focal disagreement. As the disagreement continued, Mrs. Vandenberg attempted unsuccessfully to re-negotiate her way to the floor. KV: I hear a little bit of disagreement. Sh. Tomika: Up there you said not counting the pegs. You said not counting the pegs between the back and over THERE. Hannah: No you don’t! It’ s not a rule about that. No you don’t! Rather than taking up Mrs. Vandenberg’s comment, several students continued to talk, among them Hannah, Joanne, Tomika, and Braden. Tomika continued her disagreement with Braden. At the same time, Hannah became more vocal about the interaction in which she was involved. ”No you don't. It's not a rule about that. No you don't,” Hannah declared about something I couldn't yet detect, her voice getting louder and making this second disagreement evident. Perhaps seeking to understand what was going on, perhaps to rein in the discussion, Mrs. Vandenberg asked, KV: Tell me what you're DISAGREEING about. Right now Ijust hear comments Hannah: Right now we’re disagreeing because, KV: Hannah. Hannah: (continuing) Joanne and Ali keep saying that a, a geoboard you HAVE to count the squares not the pegs. Sts and Joanne: No. Nuh uh. 160 Without hesitation Hannah turned to face her teacher and spoke for the girls, essentially saying, ”Right now we're disagreeing because, Joanne and Ali keep saying that a, a geoboard you HAVE to count the squares not the pegs." The girls in question quickly said ”no" and ”nuh uh.” Still at the table and facing Mrs. Vandenberg, Hannah turned back around to face Joanne and Ali across her table to say, ”But you don't HAVE to. If you're not-if you don't WANT to.” Responding so quickly that their speaking overlapped Hannah and Joanne interacted with rapid-fire turns: Joanne: I never said you HAVE to. Hannah: Uh huh! I said I said to you, you don't HAVE to and you goes 'yes you do.‘ Joanne: Not necessarily, but basically yeah you do. Hannah: Uh uh. There's not a rule or a LAW! Ali: That’s how you do fractions. Hannah seemed to take the firm stance that counting squares versus pegs is not cast in stone in the form of a rule or law. Her verbal and nonverbal cues indicated that she held to this conviction. She spoke more loudly, got up out of her chair, and began to walk toward the supply bins to get a geoboard, continuing to voice her disagreement as she moved across the room. After a comment from Ali which seemed to support Joanne's idea, Mrs. Vandenberg attempted to bring Steve into the disagreement, by calling on him. However, Hannah continued speaking, and her classmates began to lend support to Ali and Joanne to become a coalition of speakers (Florio & DeTar, 1995). Because one comment piled on another, I found I had to work hard to glean what they said. And in the midst of this I said-you said debate Ali spoke up, saying to 161 Hannah across the desk, ”That's how you do fractions.” Mrs. Vandenberg called on Steve, who walked from the back of the room to the overhead projector at the front. But before Steve could say anything, Hannah continued, and Mrs. Vandenberg was again distracted, this time by Michaela, who silently beckoned her to the front of the room. Facing Ali as she stood up and pushed in her chair, Hannah walked across the room to get a geoboard from a storage bin, and exclaimed, Hannah: So. We’re not DOING the fractions! KV: Sh. Sts: Yes we are! Hannah: I said what if we AREN’T! St: But we ARE! Monica: But we ARE, Hannah. That’s fractions. Hannah expressed a second view, that this geoboard work of dividing the board into halves was not fractions. Her classmates disagreed, saying that this is fractions. As Mrs. Vandenberg quickly surveyed the room and shushed the rest of the students, what began as a three-way disagreement between Hannah, Joanne, and Ali seemed to shift, becoming a disagreement between Hannah and the class. ”Yes we are [doing the fractions]!” called several students. ”I said what if we AREN’T!” responded Hannah. There she stood at the rear corner of the room, facing the entire class. ”But we ARE!" I heard someone say, unable to determine who because this disagreement both erupted and played out so quickly. ”But we ARE, Hannah. That's fractions" said a quiet voice from the front of the room, Monica 162 as it turned out, Monica who rarely spoke up during discussions. Loudly, Hannah continued, Hannah: People can count pegs on this if they want! It's not a LAW that you—that you CAN ”1‘!” She leaned toward her classmates and stepped a little closer to Monte who sat at a table near where she stood. Hannah took a geoboard from the table, turned, held it up and leaned forward as she looked toward Joanne and Ali while she emphatically spoke in a louder voice. But, the debate wasn't over: Joanne: Hannah you don't have to get so burned UP! Monte: Yeah! When Joanne said she ”doesn't have to get so burned up,” Hannah did something unusual, for this student or any other in this classroom during my nearly 50 observations - she lifted the geoboard over her head in a threatening fashion, glared at Monte, leaned toward him, and acted as though she was about to hit him it. Monte and Michaela reacted immediately, with ”Roar!” and ”Whoa!” With this uncomfortable violation of participation norms for disagreement, Mrs. Vandenberg worked to explicate them. She called Hannah to where she stood to the right of the overhead projector. In this moment, the norms of participation were violated in a way found at no other time. Somewhat indirectly, Mrs. Vandenberg focused on the violation, not directing her comments specifically to Hannah, but to the entire class. In fact, when she called Hannah to her Mrs. Vandenberg put her arm around Hannah's shoulder as she addressed the class. I watched as Hannah briefly smiled when Mrs. Vandenberg talked to all about the inappropriateness of raising one's hand or yelling at 163 someone else. But the fracas continued. ”You have to count the squares” Ali called. Presumably to nip any physical harm in the bud, Mrs. Vandenberg stopped the disagreement. KV: Time out. Time 931;. TIME OUT. Slowly and more loudly each time, Mrs. Vandenberg spoke and gestured for everyone’s attention. Her raised arm as she said this was a common signal for students' attention and quiet was the result. When she lowered her arm, I didn't hear a sound. Some students quickly resumed talking, and while Mrs. Vandenberg put her arm around Hannah's shoulder, she spoke finally and emphatically, to state the norms for participation in disagreement which KV: TIME OUT! Right now—what does time out mean?” (A child kept talking while she was saying ”Time out.”) Moira: It means ’be quiet, please!’ After Moira’s whisper-like definition, Mrs. Vandenberg explained, KV: It means be a listener. Right now I heard SO many good ideas but I had a hard time following ANY of them. I also saw one person get a little bit upset. Let's remember that we're disagreeing with each other's IDEAS, not the PEOPLE. OK? To raise your HAND to someone else or, to, YELL at someone else is not appropriate. OK? I called on Steve because Steve, looked like he had something important to say and he was so PATIENTLY raising his hand.” Stepping in during the disagreement and stopping it, Mrs. Vandenberg made clear the norms for participation in disagreement: *the children were to disagree with ideas and not people; *the children were not to raise a hand to or yell at one another. 164 Thus re-installing the disagreement norms, she appeared to imply the following rights and duties of participants: Table 2: Participant Rights and Duties Speaker 1 Speaker 2 Right: To say idea Duty: To listen to idea Duty: To listen to reasoning about Right. To disagree with idea why disagreeing Right: To clarifyc,1 articulate, defend Duty: To listen to articulation of idea 1 ea Because these norms were violated by the inappropriateness of ” [RAISING] a hand to someone or, [YELLING] at someone else,” Mrs. Vandenberg intervened to repair the interaction. Then, she redirected the discussion to what Steve had to say, which was to count the squares. Pointing to Antonia's geoboard shape (which was still projected from the overhead), Steve said ”Um what I did was, um I counted all this. Well I changed Joanne's idea a little bit. I um, I thought I-I counted, I counted squares in this." He pointed to and counted out loud each square to the right, and then the left, of Antonia's line. Mrs. Vandenberg then targeted the disagreement-related issues when she said, ”So the disagreement here is whether we count the pegs or squares.” To review, similar to May 3, the math task initially under discussion on May 7 was exploratory in nature. Mrs. Vandenberg planned for this task so that the children had geoboards as an additional representation with which to think about fractions. Mrs. Vandenberg drew on a resource new to her when she developed this visual spatial exploration for the children. During students' descriptions of how they made half, the predominant notion they talked about 165 was counting pegs. When disagreements erupted, both included debate over counting pegs. Whereas Tomika questioned Braden about pegs he said he didn't count, Hannah raised the issue of whether a person had to count squares and not pegs, and if there was a law or rule saying one had to do so. Interestingly, on May Sit was Hannah who asserted that to make half on the geoboard, she should count squares. Yet here, on May 7, she argued in favor of an additional idea, as well as the notion that there was not a rule or law saying that they cannot count pegs. Hannah opened the possibility that she meant that 1/ 2 can be represented by counting squares or pegs, that there are ways of representing 1/ 2 either way. However, different from May 3, May 7 began without detail regarding math questions for the day. Whereas Mrs. Vandenberg initiated math time on May 3 with specific statements about the questions they would be discussing, who and what they discussed on April 28, directions about journal writing, and management, on May 7 she started the lesson differently. Mrs. Vandenberg invited Michaela and then Antonia to share ”an idea about the work that we have been doing with the geoboards and fractions," which they did. Michaela explained the way she counted pegs and worked with extra bands, and Antonia talked about and projected the shape she and Miss Schram connected to Ali's, also focusing on pegs as the way to think about making half on the geoboard. When Braden explained his shape, two disagreements erupted at about the same time, one between he and Tomika over which pegs he did not count, and the other initially between Hannah, Joanne, and Ali about counting pegs or squares. The latter became the focus of everyone's attention and ended with Mrs. Vandenberg's ”Time outs.” 166 From its beginnings when two girls talked about their ways to make half on geoboards to the moment when the teacher stopped the disagreement about whether there was a law about counting pegs or squares, May 7 revealed the students’ thinking about the task of making halves, which for the most part, consisted of talk about counting pegs, but also about counting squares. However, in addition to their thinking about halves on the geoboard, the disagreement prompted me to take notice of the ways in which these children interacted over each other's thinking. Similar to May 3, on May 7 Mrs. Vandenberg again stopped the episode of disagreement. However, the reason she stopped it was not math-related but due to negative and inappropriate interactions between children. Instead of creating questions to further the discussion (as on May 3), to stop this episode she re- stated the norms of disagreement: T: Let's remember that we're disagreeing with each other's IDEAS, not the PEOPLE. OK? To raise your HAND to someone else or, to, YELL at someone else is not appropriate. OK? Then, she concluded these comments by saying the behavior-related reason she called on Steve: ”I called on Steve because Steve, looked like he had something important to say and he was so PATIENTLY raising his hand.” These post-disagreement norms that Mrs. Vandenberg explicated leaned toward focusing on a classmate's idea in disagreement, rather than attacking them as a person, physically or by yelling at them. She also implied that in order to do this, these children must be polite. She seemed to be getting at disagreeing with each other's ideas, not the people; but also disagreeing in respectful, civil ways. 167 What is the Disagreement About? Whereas the disagreements discussed in Chapter Five were about fractions content, the content in this disagreement was lost to the negative interactions between children. Yes, they evidenced their involvement in the disagreement and in their ideas, but the very act of disagreeing quickly overrode the math content. Lack of social competence prevented academic competence from playing its role. Mathematically, the disagreement on May 7 between Tomika and Braden seemed to be over what Braden said not to count. Tomika's position was that Braden said not to count pegs to make his halves, then said to count them. Noting this, Braden revised and said he meant not counting particular (i.e., middle) pegs. The longer and more prominent disagreement between Hannah, Joanne, and Ali appeared on first glance to be over counting pegs or squares, whether Joanne said ”you have to,” and if there was a ”rule or law.” However, the underlying position which Hannah took was that 1 / 2 can be represented either by counting pegs or by counting squares. And yet, beyond the mathematics in the disagreement were the pronounced interactions between children, and the features of these interactions that served to distance their competing ideas. The Interactions of the thldren Although these fourth graders evidenced their involvement in the disagreement on May 7, it took a negative turn which resulted in the need for this teacher to step in for interactional and not math-related reasons. Discomfort in disagreement (Lampert, Rittenhouse, & Crumbaugh, 1993) for these children and their teacher (and me) evidenced itself as an unanticipated factor. Similar to 168 May 3, Mrs. Vandenberg again stopped the disagreement. However, her reasons for doing so on May 7 were starkly different. Close examination of student interactions shed light on why she may have needed to do this, as well as why the episode took on a negative tone. During the episode, the children talked louder, used ambiguous pronouns, and shifted to a use of negatives, such as ”didn’t," ”don’t,” and ”aren’t.” And, to support her cause, Hannah called in an outside expert. However, rather than convince others of her stand on the issue of counting pegs or squares, Hannah ended up distancing herself from her classmates. First, the children spoke more loudly and emphatically during disagreement than they did on May 3. For example, Tomika turned up the volume and emphasized particular words when she disagreed with Braden about the pegs he said not to count (”the ones in the middle”). Tomika: But you said up THERE not counting the pegs. Braden: I mean not counting the pegs in the middle. Tomika: But you didn't SAY in the middle. You're talking about right THERE. You're talking about right THERE in the middle. Similarly, Hannah, Joanne, and Ali talked loudly and emphatically, as though in an attempt to add meaning to their words: Hannah: Right now we're disagreeing because. . Joanne and Ali keep saying that a, a geoboard you HAVE to count the squares not the pegs. Hannah: But you don't HAVE to. If you're not—if you don't WANT to. 169 Joanne: I never said you HAVE to. Hannah: Uh huh! I said I said to you you don't HAVE to and you goes 'yes you do.‘ Joanne: Not necessarily, but basically yeah you do. Hannah: Uh uh. There's not a rule or a LAW! Ali: That's how you do fractions. T: Steve. Hannah: So, we're not DOING the fractions! T: Shh. Sts: Yes we are! Hannah: I said what if we AREN'T! St: But we ARE! Monica: But we are, Hannah. That's fractions. Hannah: People can count pegs on this if they want! It's not a LAW that you—that you CAN 'T! Joanne: Hannah you don't have to get so burned UP! In addition to an amplified use of voice in disagreement, these students increasingly used pronouns. However, unlike May 3 when they to used "you” either to refer to a particular classmate or to the group generally, on May 7, I frequently questioned to whom they were talking when they used ”you," especially problematic during whole-group discussion with 26 children in the room. Were they talking to someone in particular, or not? Their generalized use of ”you” appeared to function to say, when any person does this kind of problem, then they have to use this method. For example, when Hannah initially said, ”Joanne and Ali keep saying that a, a geoboard you HAVE to count the squares not the pegs,” She was looking at Mrs. Vandenberg when she said this about the squares. Paradoxically, it would seem that Joanne and Ali just told 170 Hannah about counting squares, but here Hannah said ”you.” By contrast, when Joanne responded, ”. . . but basically yeah you do," she was looking at, therefore presumably talking to, Hannah. For the rest of the disagreement, Hannah continued to look toward Joanne and Ali, apparently to underscore who she talked to. But I noticed something else unique to Hannah's address as she developed her case for counting pegs: she shifted from the use of ”you” to "we," and others followed suit: Hannah: We're not DOING the fractions. T: Shh. Sts: Yes we are! Hannah: I said what if we AREN'T! St: But we ARE! Monica: But we are, Hannah. That's fractions. Perhaps Hannah’s use of ”we" was her effort to re-establish a sense of community after she failed with her I-versus-you stance. The sense of community purported by ”we" would go farther to convince others of her position on rules about counting squares (or, more precisely, that there is no rule that they had to, nor a rule that they couldn’t count pegs). Others indicated their involvement when they called out comments to Hannah and also used "we." They, too, seemed interested in the notions of squares, pegs, and fractions. Then, as though building her case from the personal (you) to the larger ”we,” Hannah seemed to bring an outside authority into this classroom: rules or laws behind what goes on in math. And bringing in the outside authority allowed Hannah to call on ”them” to bolster her case against her classmates. Overall, their pronoun 171 use unfolded in the following order, showing how the children spoke in a you said/ I said fashion, then shifted to we are / we aren’t, to the more general ”we" and ”people," and concluded with the very personal, "Hannah, you don’t have to get so burned up:” You said (T omika) I meant (Braden) You M say (T omika) You're talking about (T omika) No you M1 (Hannah) Right now we're disagreeing about (Hannah) Joanne and Ali keep saying (Hannah) You have to (Hannah) But you Mt have to. . if you w want to (Hannah) PQNQWPQN!‘ 10. Im said (Joanne) 11. I said (Hannah) 12. to you (Hannah, to Joanne) 13. basically yeah you do (Joanne) 14. That's how you do fractions (Ali) 15. We're not doing fractions (Hannah) 16. Yes we are (students) 17. I said (Hannah) 18. what if we geluHannah) 19. But we are (student) 20. But we are (Monica) 21. People can (Hannah) 22. Hannah you don't have to (Joanne) 172 The students’ use of ”you," ”I,” ”we,” and ”people can,” evolved into an overall adversarial stance in the episode, especially when combined with the multiple instances of negatives. Tomika, Hannah, and Joanne inserted words such as ”don't,” ”don't have to," and "I never said,” as though to differentiate their thinking from someone else's. The I-you, I never said-I said, we're not-we are, you have to-you don't have to position-taking of the children created a formula to breach communication about math, in fact to result in talking past, rather than to, one another. In addition, it elevated not only the personal nature of the disagreement, it also distanced Hannah interactionally from her classmates. This differed from the children’s interactions on May 3 during the episode of disagreement, when fractions content was at the fore (although not to the extent as during single disagreements, as discussed in Chapter Five), amplifying the difference between the episodes on May 3 and May 7. On May 3, the children also spoke loudly, but not to the extent of May 7. And they also used pronouns, but on May 3 the pronouns had fairly direct referents to make clear to what or whom the children referred. What especially distinguished the May 7 episode from the episode of May 3 were the negatives used by these children, Hannah’s reference to rules or laws and the very personal, and negative, tone which the disagreement took on. The following table compares children's pronoun choice and use of negatives on these two days and highlights this difference: 173 Table 3: Student Pronoun Use on May 3 and May 7 May 3 Episode May 7 Episode 1. you ga_n't solve it (Monte) 1. You said (Tomika) 2. You can make that into a fraction 2. I meant (Braden) (Joanne) 3. You didn't say (Tomika) 3. I don't think it would be a fraction 4. You're talking about (T omika) (Hannah) 5. No you don't (Hannah) 4. You would have to take pieces out 6 Right now we're disagreeing about (Hannah) 5. I'll show you (Hannah) 6. if you shaded one in (Hannah) 7. if you shaded in (Hannah) 8. Or you could say 'one.’ (Ali) 9. if you just put it in half (Hannah) 10. if you colored in half (Hannah) 11. But you would still split up (Jody) 12. And you color the other side (Hannah) 13. Or you can just put one (Ali) 14. Why would you put one (Hannah) 15. Ali I want you to think (Hannah) 16. she's just said (Shawna) 17. you can do it (Shawna) 18. You could call it (Hannah) 19. what would you call (Hannah) 20. Icall it (Austin) 21. What would you call (Hannah) 22. Icall it (Austin) 23. What would you call (Hannah) (Hannah) 7. Joanne and Ali keep saying (Hannah) 8. You have to (Hannah) 9. But you don't have to. . if you don't want to (Hannah) 10. I never said (Joanne) 11. I said (Hannah) 12. to you (Hannah, to Joanne) 13. basically yeah you do (Joanne) 14. That's how you do fractions (Ali) 15. We're not doing fractions (Hannah) 16. Yes we are (students) 17. I said (Hannah) 18. what if we aggrit (Hannah) 19. But we are (student) 20. But we are (Monica) 21. People can (Hannah) 22. Hannah you 51931 have to (Joanne) 174 The third indication from these students of the episode's negative tone was their lack of connection to each other's ideas. By contrast to their interactions on May 3, their ideas on May 7 faded so that the children seemed to step up for a face-off, to separate, rather than connect, thinking, and to compete rather than collaborate over math ideas. In this episode, the civility Mrs. Vandenberg fostered faded along with the math focus. The Role of the Teacher In some ways, Mrs. Vandenberg's interactions with the children to begin the lessons were similar on May 7 and May 3. For example, like the children, during the episode of disagreement on May 7, Mrs. Vandenberg also talked louder and with emphasis. She also orchestrated the discussion by connecting students’ ideas, as in the following examples: T: Oh. This is what happened when we worked with ALI'S idea. Remember? Every TIME people would change it we ended up back where we started with the halfs down the middle. What do you guys think of Michaela's idea? T: JOANNE had a good point. she said, 'but MICHAELA you had gaps in the middle.’ She said, 'And it wasn't evenly.’ And, in addition to connecting ideas during the discussion, Mrs. Vandenberg also laid the discursive foundation for disagreement through questions and comments: T: OK. We counting pegs or squares inside? T: So Michaela when YOU divided up the rectangle, in order to have 2 equal parts you had to get RID of a nail. Is that, is THAT what you're saying? T: Is THIS piece equal to THIS piece? 175 However, on May 7, Mrs. Vandenberg did not insert comments and questions as frequently as she did on May 3. Leading up to May 3's episode, Mrs. Vandenberg's speaking tums were to ask questions and make connections across children's contributions. However, prior to the episode on May 7, her connections and questions to orchestrate the discussion occurred less often and were less specific. Also in contrast to May 3, before and during the episode on May 7, Mrs. Vandenberg was forced to spend considerable energy on management. For example, while Michaela and Antonia were at the overhead describing their geoboard designs, Mrs. Vandenberg frequently had to shush the rest of the children and remind them to listen to the girls. Different from the details she laid out on May 3, math time on May 7 seemed not as strategically orchestrated. Lost in the episode were the norms of disagreement Mrs. Vandenberg fostered all year, and the indexed (Mehan, 1979) classroom rule that children were to disagree with civility and respect. On May 7, the second disagreement (first with Joanne, then with others) seemed weighted down by the personal and negative tone which overtook it. This episode seemed to take the discussion into a downward spiral. Rather than stopping the disagreement (but not children's thinking) to sustain thinking about one or more math ideas from the children, and potentially further disagreement, on May 7, Mrs. Vandenberg extinguished disagreement to also end inappropriate behavior. She reminded students that disagreement was supposed to be about ideas, that they were to be listeners, and that it was not appropriate to raise a hand to someone else or to yell at them. In addition, when she subsequently called on Steve, she specifically stated the behavior-related reason 176 why she called on him: ”he looked like he had something important to say and he was so patiently raising his hand." The episode of disagreement on May 7 stands in contrast to other disagreements discussed in this study. During this episode, the children frequently competed for the floor, and once they negotiated their way onto it, competed to sustain an idea. Negotiation of meaning became less pressing than who and what idea had the floor. The children were certainly involved, but more so in conflict than in disagreement of the sort Mrs. Vandenberg fostered. The previously-unified dialogical relation (Burbules, 1993) and fractions content so prevalent in the disagreements discussed in Chapter Five and in the episode on May 3 were overrun by competition and distance, which Mrs. Vandenberg repaired when she stopped the disagreement. 177 CHAPTER SEVEN CONCLUSIONS AND IMPLICATIONS Summary and Discussion This exploratory study was an examination of student-to-student address in disagreement during whole group discussion, those moments when these fourth graders attempted to independently maintain the norms of mathematical disagreement fostered by Mrs. Vandenberg. In these instances, these children independently engaged in a subset of the mathematical discourse similar to what mathematicians do: they disagreed. This study is timely in that it looked at mathematical discourse advocated by reform documents (National Council of Teachers of Mathematics, 1989, 1991 ; National Research Council, 1989), to gain increased understanding of its complexity. Focusing on the sub-question, What do these students need to learn to be involved in disagreement, it reveals what these children needed to learn how to participate in mathematical discourse, and more specifically, mathematical disagreement, with its norms different than in traditional math classrooms. And in so doing, there were two competencies the students had to learn and demonstrate which contributed to the nature of the disagreements: academic competence (knowledge of fractions) and social competence (how to respectfully and appropriately disagree). However, there was more they needed to learn. These children also needed to learn to use these competencies in synchrony to mathematically disagree. Mathematically, they had to sort out the legitimacy of assertions, and as they occurred, weigh them in the face of the assumptions, stated reasoning and justification behind them. And when they mathematically disagreed, they were to do it respectfully, to give 178 evidence for their position while saving face (Lampert, Rittenhouse 8r Crumbaugh, 1996). Whereas, in the traditional math lesson the teacher controls the discourse and does most of the talking, in lessons such as these where the teacher planned an open-ended task with discussion, it is envisioned that teachers will involve students in discourse. There are different expectations of and demands on teachers and students. For teachers, this adds a layer of complexity to classroom discussion and disagreement which may be unnerving, because not only is the task open-ended, so is the discussion. This raises new questions for teachers, who may be unsure about whether to manage the mathematics, the social nature of who is disagreeing and in what ways, or both. And, it raises an additional issue for teachers. Because teachers tend to be insecure both with the subject matter of mathematics as well as the teaching of it, they may naively assume that there is at least one aspect of the new mathematical discourse with which they can succeed: discussion. After all, we "discuss” all the time. However, participating in discussion and orchestrating it are very different, as this study revealed. This study shows that managing the interactional aspect of these disagreements may be as demanding as managing the mathematical aspect, shedding light on the fact that the combination of both aspects, mathematical and interactional, results in something greater than its parts, generative mathematical disagreement. Focusing on a second sub-question, What is the role of this teacher and these students during disagreements, this study also underscores the strategic role of this teacher in the successful management of disagreement during discourse of this nature. As this teacher fostered norms of mathematical 179 discourse, these fourth graders developed academic and social competencies, to learn to mathematically disagree about fractions. This is demanding and complex teacher work, and in the words of O’Connor (1996), ”is not for the faint hearted” (p. 496). This study reveals in Chapter Five that when Mrs. Vandenberg momentarily let out the tether, the children negotiated the floor to directly and publicly disagree with one another and could do so with academic and social competence. However, this study also shows how, in episodes (especially on May 7), the interactions of the children overwhelmed their mathematical, or academic, competence. Without their teacher to manage these longer disagreements, students were unable to continue the collaborative norms of disagreement she fostered. In episodes of disagreement, the fractions content on the floor collided with individuals’ faltering abilities to express them with one another, and the disagreement as an educative discourse phenomenon disintegrated. The disagreeing students clearly demonstrated their commitment to their ideas and to the discourse when they continued to disagree without Mrs. Vandenberg. However, without the equally important social competence in the disagreement, academic competence was lost in the fray. This study differs from other studies of disagreement because it examined disagreement during whole-group discussion. There was much discursive responsibility shouldered by Mrs. Vandenberg to orchestrate discussion with 26 fourth graders in the room participating in whole group. From the beginning to its conclusion, she drew children and their ideas to the public discourse, asked questions, connected ideas, and inserted task- and behavior-related reminders while navigating the fractions terrain. Studies of disagreement in small groups, either without the involvement of the teacher or with limited involvement, do 180 not place on the shoulders of teachers these moment-to-moment demands with the entire class. In this setting, disagreement varied in its influence on the discourse. When the mathematical and interactional aspects of student disagreements were tightly woven, the mathematical disagreement contributed to the discussion and propelled it forward. And, when the disagreement was laden by the interactional aspect, the disagreement created a gap in the discussion, and in the episode of disagreement on May 7, forced its shutdown. On that day, in spite of Mrs. Vandenberg’s established norms for respectful disagreement, the autonomy of disagreeing children got in the way of educative disagreement. Their way of disagreeing subordinated the math content. Rather than construction of joint meaning, they engaged in the destruction of joint meaning (0’ Connor, 1996). At times (for example, on May 3 during the episode when the children debated whether ”1 /2” was ”one-half,” ”one-two,” or ”one-secon ”) when disagreement began, the children seemed to be negotiating meaning about fractions. Although at first glance these children appeared to be disagreeing about what to name particular fractions, there was additional meaning behind their statements. I believe they conceptually explored what a fraction was (i.e., part of a large number, part of one whole) and in doing so, created drawings and labels to clarify this thinking. In the episodes, the talk was more from the world of these children, more theirs than the mathematical discourse Mrs. Vandenberg fostered. The world of the child met the world of mathematics. That is, their language and ways of interacting took precedence over what they were learning in this fourth grade. Let me illustrate with two examples, first of these children’s language and second, of their ways of interacting. Regarding the language of 181 these children, on May 3 while struggling with what to name the numeric fraction, 1 / 2, their invented terms, ”one-two” and "one-second," bumped into the conventional mathematical term, one-half (inserted in the episode by Mrs. Vandenberg). In the words of Vygotsky (1978), the moment seemed to be a time when the children attempted to create schooling’s scientific concept (”one-half”) on their own. In the moment, the children seemed to let go of what Vygotsky termed spontaneous, non-school concepts of childhood (”one-two” and "one- second”). Second, regarding their ways of interacting, on May 7 when Mrs. Vandenberg receded, the fourth graders continued to disagree when she stepped aside to confer privately with Shawna. In this disagreement, there was no one to manage the desired norms of respectful disagreement, and the autonomy of the disagreeing children shone through. Rather than disagreeing about the merits of counting pegs or squares on the geoboard, the children disagreed about who said what and to whom. The relationship between the purpose, function, and mathematical import of the disagreements led to the examination of mathematical tasks which fueled mathematical thinking. For example, the partnered task on May 11" which led to the creation and discussion on May 12 of Kim’s Diamond prompted extended and strategic reasoning. For much of discussion on May 12, Kim’s Diamond held the floor. These children teased apart several fractions ideas: equal area (do two diamonds equal one square) and, in relationship to the whole represented by the diamond, what did the shaded square represent and how do you know? '7 You will recall that this partnered task was on a handout, on which the children were to think of a fraction and do the following: Draw a picture that represents your fiaction. Explain in your journal. Then, on the bottom half of the paper, Mrs. Vandenberg wrote: What fraction does the above picture represent? Why? How did you solve this? 182 Together, the children nudged one another’s thinking, to determine answers to these questions. And listeners also appeared involved in this and most disagreements, as evidenced by their spontaneous call outs to join in the disagreement; and as evidenced by their attentiveness (e. g., looking at the representations magnified from the overhead, looking at the speakers). Kim's Diamond as an example of an offshoot of this partnered task demonstrates that mathematical disagreements can serve an important function in the discourse: to get at the underpinnings of another’s assumptions and at the reasoning behind their position. This example stands in contrast to the episode of May 3 when several ideas stood in competition on the floor, or on May 4, when Jonah’s geoboard drawing of one-half was corrected by Jody. As discussed earlier, ways of getting math ideas on the floor during episodes of disagreement and ways of talking to each other were negotiated by the children. When Mrs. Vandenberg receded during episodes, they, rather their teacher, appeared to be managing it (however unsuccessfully). The children seemed to act in situ to define the content and direction of this segment of the discourse. This study shows that disagreement provided a window into these children’s developing fractions knowledge. Whereas the single disagreements were wrapped more tightly around fractions content due to Mrs. Vandenberg’s management of them, the episode of disagreement on May 3 teetered away from content, in part because she inadvertently receded from her role as discourse conductor. And, this study demonstrates that when this teacher receded, for children disagreement (e.g., in the episode of May 7) can be competitive, frustrating and uncomfortable (Lampert, Rittenhouse, & Crumbaugh, 1996). 183 However, what is gained from examination of this difficult disagreement, in combination with that gained from the examination of them all, is that disagreement can be respectful and collaborative. After all, competition in the classroom as a positive motivator is good only for the winners. Analyses of disagreement in Chapter Six demonstrate that, if the authors of reform documents (and teacher educators who teach in the spirit and context of reform) have as their goal for students to own their ideas and increase mathematical power through discourse, a caution is in order. We cannot assume that the process by which these ideas are owned is easily achievable or a positive experience. This is a process that requires previously under-recognized teacher knowledge and skill, so that the educative value and complexity of this discourse is appreciated. Regarding the way in which fractions content and social interactions were separated for examination in this study, of course, the act of disagreeing over an aspect of fractions content is unified and simultaneous. In real time during a discussion, there is not one or the other, the content or the process at work. Each constitutes the other. However, when examined separately for the purpose of this study, the two competencies, mathematical (i.e., academic) and interactional (i.e., social), provide new insights into the importance of both in order for children to mathematically disagree with respect and civility while saving face. I do not claim that learning takes place only during disagreement. My claim is that in disagreement during discussion, teachers are afforded a window into students’ thinking, a useful opportunity if teachers wish to foster the mathematical activity and learning advocated by the authors of reform 184 documents. Disagreement is a moment at which competing (or, colliding) ideas have the potential to force children to explain, defend, and justify ideas. Disagreement is also a moment which may be seized by teachers and capitalized upon to increase student involvement in discussion. This would assist teachers in their efforts to enhance students’ mathematical power, as called for in reform documents (NCTM, 1989, 1991 ; NRC, 1989). As such, disagreement is a learning nexus from which teachers can learn in the moment what students think and why. Learning does not require disagreement, but mathematical disagreement is a public, visible, and spontaneous site in which to gauge student learning of fractions content and learning of the requisite social competence by which content is negotiated. And, teacher reflection on the relationship between task and disagreement can lead to a generative use of disagreement. The findings of this study, discussed above, draw attention to some of richness of disagreement in mathematical discourse. In addition, it emphasizes challenges to teachers and students when disagreements arise during mathematics discussions. I now turn to implications for theory and practice. Theory and Practice Theory With heightened interest in mathematics teaching and learning that is aligned with reform documents (NCTM, 1989, 1991 ; NRC, 1989) this study of disagreement during discussions is timely. It is timely because mathematical discourse of the type advocated by authors of reform is complex with much yet to be learned about the new roles and responsibilities of teachers and students. Studies of student disagreement and argument, although emerging, are rare (see, 185 for example, Cobb & Yackel, 1994; Lampert, Rittenhouse, & Crumbaugh, 1996; Wilkinson 8: Martino, 1993). When combined with current interest in sociocultural learning theory and its basic premise that language use mediates learning, this study reveals that research on the ways in which children and teachers negotiate meaning through subject matter discourse is worthwhile. The process by which meaning is negotiated is situation specific and not to be prescribed. The advocated discourse increases students' role in the construction of their understandings and draws upon disputes over ideas as ways to refine and develop ideas. However, studies which simultaneously draw on language-based theory and use method grounded in the language use of participants in order to examine discourse- based mathematics teaching, and disagreement in particular, are rare. Controversies of the kind that the Standards (1991) seem to promote and were examined in this study are not common in most classrooms; to the contrary, student consensus and acceptance are more the rule. More work which draws in tandem on language and meaning-based perspectives is called for to better understand student disagreement during mathematics discussions. In this study I have attempted to show the interrelationship between sociocultural learning theory, techniques of discourse analysis from the method of sociolinguistics, and the study of students’ disagreement during fourth-grade mathematics. This study underscores the complexities of teaching and learning in the context of reform. The results of my study reveal that there is much for teachers and students to attend to while negotiating meaning during mathematics discussions. While subject matter is central, my study shows that there is more to mathematical disagreement than mathematics. 186 The analyses reveal that these children needed to learn how to participate in mathematical discourse and more specifically, mathematical disagreement, with its different norms than in traditional math classrooms. And, in learning how to participate in this discourse, there were interdependent competencies students had to learn which contributed to the nature of the disagreements: academic competence (knowledge of fractions) and social competence (how to respectfully and appropriately disagree). And in addition (and more importantly), these fourth graders needed to learn to use these competencies in synchrony to mathematically disagree. The analyses also show that the focus of disagreements hinges on the role of the teacher in the successful management of disagreement. There are norms to be fostered to promote a dialogical relation (Burbules, 1993), norms which lay the foundation for the social construction of subject-matter knowledge. These norms are also central in the formation of academic and social competencies required to learn to mathematically disagree over subject-matter content. The analyses show that disagreement varied in its function in the discourse. When the mathematical and interactional aspects of students’ disagreements were tightly woven, the mathematical disagreement appeared to contribute to the discussion and propel it forward. And, when the disagreement was laden by the interactional aspect, the disagreement created a gap in the discussion, and in one instance of disagreement, forced its shutdown. The relationship between the purpose, function, and mathematical significance of the disagreements led to the examination of mathematical tasks which initially fueled thinking. Mathematical disagreements can serve an important function in the discourse: that is, to get at the underpinnings of 187 another’s assumptions and at the reasoning behind their position. This stands in contrast to other disagreements in the data which pointed out a flaw in a drawn representation; or, in one episode, when several ideas stood in competition on the floor. This study shows that disagreement provided a window through which to spontaneously assess children’s developing fractions knowledge. Whereas some disagreements were focused on fractions content due to Mrs. Vandenberg" management of it, one episode of disagreement veered away from content when Mrs. Vandenberg inadvertently receded from her role as discourse conductor. What encompasses these findings is the focus on situated language use and sense-making of participants. Recent research cuts across sociocultural learning theory, sociolinguistics, and mathematical discourse (see for example, Lampert, Rittenhouse, 8: Crumbaugh, 1996; O’ Connor, 1996). However, additional studies are warranted. Individually and in combination, sociocultural learning theory and the method of sociolinguistics were used to shed light on what disagreement was in this setting, in the subject matter of fourth-grade mathematics. 132191132 For educators, this study is useful, because it provides an examination of how disagreements arise, are managed, and frequently, capitalized upon. Through this lens, educators may gain insight into ways in which to plan tasks for disagreement, recognize one as it develops, and use it to gauge learners’ thinking in the moment, and to sustain a discursive course toward learning. From it, my goal is for educators to recognize such signals as when referents 188 begin to become unclear, when negatives begin to seep into the discourse, and when I said-you said talk verges on personal rather than mathematical. Increased understanding of disagreement will assist teacher educators’ efforts to heighten preservice teachers’ understanding of classroom discourse, so that they may, to the extent possible, prevent uncomfortable situations. Preservice teachers require support in their efforts to learn to foster discourse about math ideas, to become users of respectful discourse and disagreement. Limitations This study examined whole-group discussion and disagreement only. A comparison of student involvement in small- and whole-group discussion would have added insight into children’s interactions, to determine if they disagreed in the same way in both forums. The study would be stronger with greater understanding about who disagreed during small group to determine if norms in small group differed in the absence of the teacher. Without Mrs. Vandenberg, would their small-group disagreement be similar to the episodes? Analyses of additional types of data also would have strengthened this study. Predominantly, whole-group discussion was examined. Although listening and writing (journal, picture drawing) were a part of this study, They were not extensively analyzed. This study of student-to-student disagreement required an examination of discussion, and was supported by students’ journal writing. This study did not set out to examine issues of gender during disagreement. Many studies exist of adult male-female interaction during informal conversation (see for example, Tannen, 1990, 1993; West & Garcia, 1988; West & Zimmerman, 1983; Murray, 1985; Maltz 8: Borker, 1982). Studies also 189 show the ways in which teachers interact in American classrooms more often with boys than girls (e. g., Brophy, 1985). With recent reform efforts calling for increased classroom discourse as a powerful process through which to develop and enhance students’ mathematical power, studies of boy-girl interaction during classroom discourse is warranted. Such studies during disagreement, moments which can (as shown in this study) be uncomfortable and frustrating, are especially timely. Although gender-related studies of classroom discourse and disagreement are an area of potential, they were not the goal here. When examined, disagreements may be revealed as a rich source for understanding the complexities of mathematics teaching and learning. As alluded to in the Standards (1991), disagreement embedded within discourse may be a window for teachers into students’ mathematical learning and at the same time force children to articulate and justify their reasoning, for themselves and for others. In addition, a give and take may result as different ideas make their way to the floor. In the process, children may also learn respect for each others’ thinking. Questions for Future Study When student disagreement arises during whole-group discussion, it is imperative that educators (and reformers) have some sense of this complex discourse process in which students are expected to gain mathematical power, and to recognize what students need to learn in order to be involved disagreement. This study looked closely at a specific component of discourse, student disagreement, for the purpose of understanding what it takes to be involved in it in this classroom. 190 Although this study generated findings toward a better understanding of the territory of mathematical disagreement, it also raised questions about disagreement and tasks which foster mathematical disagreement: ‘What does it take to create a classroom where children disagree with respect? *How do we develop a way to distinguish educative disagreements from those that are less mathematically worthwhile? "What does it take to have mathematically worthwhile disagreements? *How do we create a context that increases the likelihood that disagreement has mathematical significance and is productive of student learning? These questions extend my intent to increase understanding of students’ disagreement during mathematics discussions. Where the goal is changes in the teaching and learning of mathematics in order to increase students’ mathematical power, studies developed to address these questions are necessary. Such studies are necessary to expand researcher knowledge of what it actually means for teachers to promote discourse with the goal of increased mathematical power. And perhaps more importantly, studies are warranted to expand researcher understanding of what it means for students to be involved in disagreement. 191 APPENDICES 192 APPENDIX A , 193 APPENDIX A Data Collected, 1992-1993 Field Notes Video Tape: Video fipe: AudiofiTape Student Math Class Fractions Unit Interviews September 25 October 1 April 28 Austin Hannah October 1 October 6 May 3 April 13 December 3 October 8 October 8 May 7 October 13 October 13 May 10 Braden Monica October 27 November 5 May 11 Monica February 23 November 17 November 24 May 12 Monte December 1 November 17 May 13 April 26 Jody January 26 November 18 May 19 March 2 February 2 November 25 Elizabeth December 1 Discussion Antonia Fractions, December 3 about May 3 pre- and post- December 8 discussions: 'n rvi : January 12 June 9 Braden January 26 Monica January 28 Teacher Monte February 2 Interviews: Elizabeth February 9 July 2 Rob February 23 July 13 Connie March 2 Antonia March 15 March 16 March 24 April 23 April 26 194 APPENDIX B 195 APPENDIX B Analyses of Discourse 828585 30.5.2053 \ was 82> :08 003033. ‘IHI 53 05 5 «amigo L0 883% 380— 2 00303 ‘l..\|||| €008. “Sega; ‘IIII \ chfioouwmmfi Saga 280— 0. 3mng 05 @088. poggom ‘ \ 0280030 05 60¢ Eofimflwmmmu :08 386.; - s \ .50: :0 $3 .0 was 05 5:5 3082586 :08 08:5 _ . \ ass wcéoaom 256 All «30% ~§§§m§u 0.5. m. BS: 0874 \ 029.38% a: S Eofioawaflu 008E3— TII 2053:8030» flcoufim poEEaxm TI Emfiofiwamfi :08 .0 809:6 0056.880 ‘ll. 005003 3 30945 0030330 Tl Emfiofiwme ".0 00:85:30. .monafiwfimfi “0055.580 x93 firs 8587qu Il.’ .auuafiofiafi poo—E..— All! \ 196 APPENDIX C 197 APPENDIX C Fractions Unit Overhead Questions April 28. Qgesg'ons to begin glass: What is a fraction? When have you used fractions? How are fractions represented? Draw and label pictures. Questions during class: None. Qtestiens at the end ef gess: Do both of these show half? Why or why not? Show other ways to represent 1/ 2. May 3. mesfiogs te begin class: Same as April 28. MW fiss: Is this 1/ 2, 2/ 4, or both? Why? WW: None. May 4. mestions to begin class: How many ways can you show halves (1 / 2)? Practice on geoboards. Record on paper. Write one explanation in journal, explaining how you can prove your idea shows a half. W m: None. Questions at the end ef class: Here’s Ali’s 2 pieces. How can we tell if these are equal amounts? May 5. mestions to begin glass: Sharing ways to make a half. From yesterday, with geoboards. Which are congruent? Which are equal? Megs during class: Respond to this in your journal: Is this [Ali’s way] a way to divide the geoboard in half? Why or why not? mestiogs at the end ef glass: None. May 7. Qeestions to begin class: None. @estiogs during Qess: Finding ways to show 1 / 2 and 2 / 4 on the geoboard. Record it on dot paper. Prove one of your ways. Questions at the end of class: None. May 10. glenestions to begin class: Sharing responses from May 7 [homework]. Where do you find fractions? How do you represent fractions (1 / 2, 2/ 4, 7/ 14, 4 / 4)? Questions during glass: Does a fraction have to be less than 1? Why? Questions et the end of class: Draw a representation for 3/5. Do the pieces have to be equal? Why? 198 May 11. gflestions to mgin glass: Draw a picture that represents a fraction and then explain it in your journal. Questions dering eless: None. Quesg'ees at the end of glass: Is 8/ 4 the same as 4/ 8? May 12. Questiens to begin class: What fraction does this picture [Kim’s diamond shape with one square colored in] represent? Why? W gees: What is 1/2 + 1/2? Why? Draw a picture of this. WW eggs: Prove that 1/ 2 is the same as 2/ 4. May 13. Questions to mgt'n eless: Where is 1/ 2 on the number line? Qdestjefi during class: Is 1/ 2 less than one? Why? WW dess: Is 1/ 4 greater than or smaller than 1 / 2? Where is it on the number line? May 19. mestions to begin class: Put in order from smallest to largest: 6/ 6, 3 / 6, 1 / 2. Draw a representation of each. Explain your solution. Qdesdegs dddng glass: None. Questions at the end of eless: None. 199 APPENDIX D 200 APPENDIX D Discourse Pattern 05F! :0605005 map—V0355 5000.. .2000 S000 >05 EoEooelaama ‘ llllllllllllllllllllllllll gages 853 coasts:- \8:0to h 3.32.... 4.... i :8.— 8 _ .0! 08.2.. \ \ _ \ \ _ . \ 8222 3.82 \ .8... e _ 53- 3 so? \ 300. .0002» _ £81. \ 9x320. \ F _ 8339a \ .§8e§o 9.8. \ _ _ \ \ _ _ \ _ _ \ .8: 08.0.6. £0000. \ 0. 802.8030 _ _ \ it}... 150 \ _ _ 3» 8 .23.. \ 1/ _ . 00.8308 \ .80.. 0: 00000 \ / _ 000E 00—0003 0550800 5000 000208 E... ....23 3:23 525:. \ / _ _ \ / 88. .2803," 82:8. \ .8. / _ _ 3.030030 83d \ 0. 80v. .8933 ‘ 5.28 a. 9 EB. 8.2 \. I I I / _ _ llllll I I / D \\ llllll lk _ 201 APPENDIX E 202 APPENDIX E Transcript of Episodes of Disagreement, May 3 and May 7 May 3 KV And how is THAT like 20 divided by 10, Hannah? Or TWO HUNDRED divided by 10, excuse me? Hannah I keep making my two sideways. OK. The REASON, OK. Two hundred divided by 10 is not a fraction. Hannah erases, then writes. KV Is NOT a fraction. But what YOU showed us IS a fraction. Hannah It's NOT a-it's NOT a fraction because- 09:51 Monte It CAN be a fraction Monte remains all this time at the OH. Hannah Because a FRACTION is less than one. TEN and TWO HUNDRED are not less than one. HERE is another kind of fraction. There are other kinds of fractions. THIS would be, one-fourth, if you shaded in one. Hannah draws a circular representation, divided into four sections. She points to one of the sections and records '1/4'. 10:16 KV OK. Hannah And, 203 Hannah 10:36 Hannah Hannah Jody Hannah How did you know that would be one—fourth. It would be TWO-fourths if you shaded in, two, THREE-fourths and FOUR-fourths. Hannah shades each additional section as she speaks. And ONE. Or you can say ONE. Ali says 'one' when Hannah says 'FOLIR-fourths'. How come you say one instead of four-fourths Ali. 'Cause it's, all colored in it's one, group. Hannah it was helpful to me the way you went through there and you colored a piece at a time, one-fourth, two-Did you guys see that? TWO-fourths, THREE-fourths. In other words if you just put it in HALF, and you colored in HALF, it would be one, Two. Ali says one-two; Hannah says one-half. One-HALF. It would be one-half. Hannah now has filled in each of the four sections, and drawn a new circle with a horizontal line across it: One-two is one-half. But you would STILL split up, two hundred and, And you color the OTHER side would be 204 Hannah Hannah Hannah 11:40 Hannah 'Other side' = top portion of the circle. Rather than finishing her sentence, Hannah records '2/‘2.’ ONE. TWO, halves. K V is speaking to someone; Monte colors over a section already filled in. OR you can just put ONE. NO! Why would you put ONE? 'Cause it's ALL FILLED IN. all filled in - each word spoken separately; speech is not fluid. OK I MISSED that last ques- NO! Hannah continues to draw circular representations of fourths. Hannah's last comment, was that that was FOUR-fourths. And Ali said it was ONE, because it was one whole-if we were thinking about that like a COOKIE, and you were getting FOUR-fourths, would you actually be getting one whole cookie? (calling out) Ali, Ali I want you to think. This PROBABLY could be one second, but then what would, THIS be. 205 Ali and Sts Hannah Shawna Joanne Hannah 12:34 this = 1/2 'What would this be? ' = Hannah draws while she asks this question. One-HALF. One HALF. That's the same as one—SECOND. T'here's one-half, there's one-SECOND. WHAT is the difference between those two? Hannah points to what is written on the OH: 1/2 1/2 Nothing. Nothing she's just said that you can do it either way. Oh so she's saying that it also would make sense for it to be called one-SECOND? My BROTHER told me- Since we talked about one-THIRD or one-FOURTH? You're right. That is two ways to say it and and another way that we've gotten used to saying it is one-HALF. I know what the answer is. Joanne what do YOU want to say? You COULD call it, you COULD call it- Hannah looks toward Joanne/Shawna/Ali and speaks loudly. 206 Hannah Austin Hannah Hannah Dallas 13:13 St Hannah You COULD call it, one-SECOND. But the REAL one-second is THIS. Now, er-Now, what would you call THIS? Right here? Joanne goes to the OH, joining Hannah. Hannah draws a circle and shades in the top half. Joanne reaches under the projector to get a new transparency. Hannah erases. She redraws a new circle, divides it in fourths, and shades in the upper right section, pointing to it/‘this '. I call it a HALF. A quarter, or a fourth. A quarter or a fourth? One-FOURTH. Hannah records ’onefourth' as she speaks. She continues recording, filling in a second portion of the drawing. Do you agree with that Kim? What would you call THAT? Two quarters or TWO-fourths. I call it a HALF. Wait! I'm curious here. I heard two different ANSWERS. Um, ALI said it's two-fourths, and AUSTIN said HALF. Same thing. It IS the same thing. Now what would you call THAT. 207 13:35 Hannah begins to draw, out of camera range. I want you to stop for a minute and respond to that in your journal. One second. We'll come right back to this. Can I write down your idea? FOUR-fourths, not ONE-fourth. If it was ONE-fourth it would be one. I want you to respond to this in your journal. Here's the picture Hannah drew you. OK she had divided this into four pieces, and then she colored, TWO. OK. And I want you to respond to this question: Is this 1 / 2, 2 / 4, or both? Why? Austin said one-half. Ali said—Hannah said it's TWO- FOURTHS. A couple of you said it's both. Tell me what YOU think and respond to why. And draw it-write the question in your journal please. But it has, FOUR pieces. Does that question make sense? So Ali you are starting to explain it NOW. You think because it has four pieces, you colored in two? You think it's both? Write the question please. Get started Rob. 208 KV Braden St 13:25 Joanne Suzanne Moira 13:34 Tomika Braden Tomika 13:48 May 7 Equal on both sides? Yeah. But that's counting the PEGS. WAIT a minute though! That's counting the pegs not the squares. But they're not the same size. Yeah if you put that ONE thing that's going like a Moira makes a horizontal line in the air with her finger. Steve raises his hand. But you said up THERE [points to OH] not counting the pegs. Hannah leans forward when she responds to Joanne I mean not counting the pegs in the middle. Suzanne leaves her desk to get a geoboard from the back of the room. But you didn't SAY in the middle. You're talking about right THERE. You're talking about right THERE not in the middle. Sean goes for a geoboard. 209 Tomika Hannah Hannah 14:00 Hannah Hannah Sts & Joanne I hear a little bit of disagreement. Sh. KVraises her arm, signaling for students ' attention. Her speech overlaps with Tomika's next turn. [points toward the OH] Up there you said not counting the pegs. You said not counting the pegs between the back and over THERE. No you don't It's not a rule about that. No you don't! Hannah '5 voice is louder. She leans in once again to interact with Joanne. Ali says something to Hannah. Also talking are Micah/Monte; Tamika/Braden. That's what we do with fractions. SO! Tell me what you're DISAGREEING about. Right now Ijust hear comments. Right now we're disagreeing because, Hannah turns toward K V as she speaks. Hannah. (continuing) Joanne and Ali keep saying that a, a geoboard you HAVE to count the squares not the pegs. No. Nuh uh. 210 Hannah Hannah Joanne Hannah 14:36 Hannah But you don't HAVE to. If you're not-if you don't WANT to Hannah turns back toward Joanne. As Joanne and Hannah interact their speech overlaps. Joanne/Ali/Hannah continue talking, even though K Vcalls on Steve, who then goes to the OH. I never said you HAVE to. Uh huh! I said I said to you, you don't HAVE to and you goes yes you do.’ Tamika leaves her table and joins K V near the door. Not necessarily, but basically yeah you do. Uh uh. There's not a rule or a LAW! That's how you do fractions. A student undertone of comments increases in volume. Steve. (facing Ali as she stands up and pushes in her chair) So, we’re not DOING the fractions! Hannah appears to ignore the fact that KV has called on Steve/ofi'ered the floor to him, as she continues to argue with Joanne and Ali. Steve walks to OH projector. Tamika returns to her desk. Hannah gets a geoboard from the round table in the back of the room. 211 Sts Hannah St Monica Hannah Joanne: Monte Sh. Now, the lone exchange is between Hannah and, first Joanne/Ali; but soon is between Hannah and class. Yes we are! I said what if we AREN'T! Hannah turns to face Joanne/Ali as she speaks. But we ARE! But we ARE Hannah. That's fractions. Pointing to the OH screen. KV returns to the OH. People can count pegs on this if they want! It's not a LAW that you don't have-that you CAN 'T. Hannah takes a geoboard from the table, turns, holds it up and leans forward, toward Joanne/Ali while she emphatically speaks in a louder voice. Hannah you don't have to get so burned UP! Joanne ’5 speech overlaps with Hannah ’5. Yeah! Monte makes a face as he speaks. Hannah lifts the geoboard over her head as though she might hit Monte with it. He sits about one foot in front of her. Hannah 212 Michaela E: Moira turns around and replaces the geoboard at the round table. Whoa! Jessica watched wide-eyed as Hannah swung the geoboard. Come here Hannah. You have to count the squares. Time out. Time out. Time out. (pause) TIME OUT. Right now—what does time out mean? (pause) KV raises her arm and makes a fist, again signaling for students' attention. This time, K Vspeaks more slowly and emphatically; also, she lowers her arm. The room is totally quiet for an instant. Some students resume talking. KV puts her arm around Hannah '5 shoulder. Braden, Steve, and Antonia are at the OH. It means be quiet please! Moira whispers this; I can hear her because she sits near me and the camera. It means be a listener. Right now I heard 80 many good ideas but I had a hard time following ANY of them. I also saw one person get a little bit upset. Let's remember that we're disagreeing with each other's IDEAS and not the PEOPLE. OK? To raise your HAND to someone else or, to, YELL at someone 213 else is not appropriate. OK? I called on Steve because Steve, looked like he had something important to say and he was so PATIENTLY raising his hand. Hannah smiles when KV says "I also saw one person get a little bit upset. " KV points to Steve. when she says "I called on Steve because... " The room is quiet when KV finishes saying all this. 214 BIBLIOGRAPHY 215 BIBLIOGRAPHY Ball, D. L. (1996). Integrity in teaching: Recognizing the fusion of the moral and intellectual. American Educational Research Journal, 33(1), 155-192. Ball, D. L. (1991). What’s all this talk about discourse? Arithmetic Teacher, 39(3), 4448. Ball, D. L. (1990). With an eye on the mathematical horizon (Craft paper 90-3). East Lansing: Michigan State University, National Center for Research on Teacher Education. Ball, D. L. 8: Wilson, S. M. (1996). Integrity in teaching: Recognizing the fusion of the moral and intellectual. American Educational Research Journal, 33(1), 155-192. Ball, D. L. (1993). Halves, pieces, and twoths: Constructing and using representational contexts in teaching fractions. In T. P. Carpenter, E. Fennema, 8: T. A. Romberg (Eds), Rationel ngbers: An integtetion ef regarch (pp. 157-195). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Barnes, D. (1992). From eommdnieetion tQ eurrigddm. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann. Bereiter, C. (1977, March). Mathematieal knowledgeability. Invited address presented to the SIG / Research in Mathematics Education at the Annual Meeting of the American Educatoinal Research Association, Chicago, Illinois. Brophy, J. (1985). Interactions of male and female students with male and female teachers. In L.C. Wilkinson 8: C. B. Marrett (Eds), ggendet influences in dassroom interaeden (pp. 115-142). Orlando: Academic Press, Inc. Burbules, N. C. (1993). Dialogee in teaching. New York: Teachers College Press. Cazden, C. (1988). Classroom disco_u_rse. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann. Cazden, C. (1986). Classroom discourse. InM. C. Wittrock (Ed.),L-I_ar_1deeek_et researeh en teaehing (pp. 432-463). New York: Macmillan. Corwin, R. B., Russell, S. J., 8: Tierney, C. C. (1990). gang fractioes. Sacramento, CA: California Department of Education. 216 Coulthard, M. (1988). An introduction i c ur nal i . New York: Longman Inc. Edelsky, C. (1993). Who’s got the floor? In D. Tannen (Ed.), g‘gnder end eenyersationel interam'on (pp. 189-227). New York: Oxford University Press. Erickson, F. (1986). Qualitative methods in research on teaching. In M.C. Wittrock (Ed.), Handbook of researQ on teeemng (pp. 119-161). New York: MacMillan. Florio—Ruane, S., 8: DeTar, J. (1995). Conflict and consensus in teacher candidates’ discussion of ethnic autobiography. English Education, 27(1), 11-39. Gee, J., Michaels, 5., 8: O'Connor, M. C. (1992). Discourse Analysis. In M. D. LeCompte, W. L. Millroy, 8: J. Preissle (Eds), The Handbook of Qualitative Research in Education (pp. 227-291). San Diego: Academic Press, Inc. Gumperz, J. J. (1994). Discogse strategies. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Hymes, D. (1972). On communicative competence. In J. B. Pride 8: J. Holmes (Eds), Sociolinguistics: Selected readings (pp. 269-293). Baltimore: Penguin. Lampert, M. (1992). Teaching and learning long division for understanding in school. In G. Leinhardt, R. Putnam, 8: R. A. Hattrup (Eds), Mi?! erithmetie fer methematies teeehing (pp. 221-282). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Lampert, M. (1989). Choosing and using mathematical tools in classroom discourse. In J. Brophy (Ed.), Transferming ehildren’s methematjes eddcation (Vol. 1, pp. 253-265). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Lampert, M. (1986). Knowing, doing, and teaching multiplication. Cognition and Instruction, 3, 305-342. Lampert, M., Rittenhouse, P., 8: Crumbaugh, C. (1996). Agreeing to disagree: Developing sociable mathematical discourse. In D. Olson 8: N. Torrance (Eds), Handbook ef educatien and heman development (pp. 731-764). Cambridge, MA: Blackwell Publishers. Maltz, D. N., 8: Borker, R. A. (1982). A cultural approach to male-female miscommunication. In J. Gumperz (Ed.), Language and social identity (pp. 196-216). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 217 Mehan, H. (1979). Learning lessods. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Murray, S. O. (1985). Toward a model of members’ methods for recognizing interruptions. Language in Society, 14(1), 31-40. National Council of Teachers of Mathematics. (1991). Brofessionel Stenderds f2; Teaehing Methemati cs. Reston, VA: The National Council of Teachers of Mathematics, Inc. National Research Council (1989). Evetybody eounts. Washington, DC: National Academy Press. 0’ Connor, M. C. (1996). Managing the intermental: Classroom group discussion and the social context of learning. In D. I. Slobin, J. Gerhardt, A. Kyratzis, 8: J. Guo (Eds), Ed e1 interaetion, social eontext, end lengeage: Esssays in hener of Sesen Mid-Tripp (pp. 495-509). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. O’Connor, M. C., 8: Michaels, S. (1993). Aligning academic task and participation status through revoicing: Analysis of a classroom discourse strategy, Anthropology and Education Quarterly, 24(4), 318-335. Perkins, D. N. (1986). Knowledge as design. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Pimm, D. (1990). Speaking mathematically. New York: Routledge. Sacks, H., Schegloff, E. A., 8: Jefferson, G. (1978). A simplest systematics for the organization of turn taking for conversation. In J. Schenkein (ed.), Studies in the organization ef conversatienal interectioe (pp. 7-55). New York: Academic Press. Schultz, J., Florio, S., 8: Erickson, F. (1982). Where is the floor? Aspects of the cultural organization of social relationships in communication at home and at school. In P. A. G. Gilmore (Ed.), Children in end out of sehdel: Ethnegtaphy and edueation (pp. 88—123). Washington, DC: Center for Applied Linguistics. Tannen, D. (1991). Talking voices: Remtition, dialogee, and imagery in eonversational diseeurse. New York: Cambridge University Press. Tannen, D. (1990). You just don’t understand: Women and men in conversation. New York: Ballantine Books. Tannen, D. (1993). Gender and conversatienal interaction. New York: Oxford University Press. Toulmin, S. (1958). The dses of argdment. New York: Cambridge University Press. 218 Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in meg. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Wells, G. (1993). Reevaluating the IRF sequence: A proposal for the articulation of theories of activity and discourse for the analysis of teaching and learning in the classroom. Linguistics and Education, 5, 1-37. Wertsch, J. V. (1985). Vygotsky and the social formation of mind. Cambridge: Harvard University Press. West, C., 8: Garcia, A (1988). Conversational shift work: A study of topical transitions between women and men. Social Problems, 35(5), 551-573. West, C., 8: Zimmerman, D. H. (1983). Small insults: A study of interruptions in cross-sex conversations between unacquainted persons. In B. Thorne, C. Kramarae, and N. Henley (Eds), Lang1_iageI gender and society (pp. 102- 117). Rowley, MA: Newbury House. Wilkinson L. C., 8: Martino, A. (1993). Students’ disagreements during small- group mathematical problem solving. In R. B. Davis 8: C. A. Maher (Eds), ghools, mathematics, and the world of realig (pp. 135-171). Boston: Allyn 8: Bacon. Yackel, E., 8: Cobb, P. (1994). The development of young children’s understanding of mathematical argumentation. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the American Educational Research Association, New Orleans, LA. 219 3 129301 7//6////ll//é/I/é/I/M/7/Iss