THESE l ””lllllll \\\\\\\\\\‘\‘\ll\°li\l\‘lllg\m 3 1293 0176 g This is to certify that the dissertation entitled THE INFLUENCE OF SENSATION-SEEKING AND MESSAGE CHOICE ON RESPONSES TO FEAR APPEALS presented by Judith M. Berkowitz has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for Ph . D . degree in Communlc atlon 0 %m L. Major professor Date [a [/2] 7’ MS U is an Affirmative Action/Equal Opportunity Institution 042771 LIBRARY Mlchigan'State University PLACE IN REFURN BOX to remove this checkout from your record. To AVOID FINES return on or before date due. MAY BE RECALLED with earlier due date if requested. DATE DUE MA ‘ 8 2 DATE DUE DATE DUE 01. NOV 1. 1: {big-.0 1m mu THE INFLUENCE OF SENSATION-SEEKING AND MESSAGE CHOICE ON RESPONSES TO FEAR APPEALS Judith M. Berkowitz A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PI-HLOSOPHY Department of Communication 1998 ABSTRACT THE INFLUENCE OF SENSATION-SEEKING AND MESSAGE CHOICE ON RESPONSES TO FEAR APPEALS By Judith M. Berkowitz The purpose of this investigation is to examine the influence of sensation seeking and message choice on responses to fear appeals. It is argued that sensation seeking will be positively associated with an individual’s desire to view a high threat fear appeal and that exposure to an optimal threat level will result in more positive attitudes and intentions. A factorial design with an immediate post-test and two-week follow-up was employed. There were two message threat level conditions (high or low) and two choice conditions (received choice/did not receive choice). In addition, a forced exposure (high threat/low threat) group was employed. Analyses indicate no relationship between sensation seeking and message choice. Further, results indicate that exposure to a non- desired message resulted in more positive behaviors than did exposure to a desired message. The implications of these findings are discussed. Dedication To Robin, for courage and confidence; To my family, for sustenance and faith; To Brian and Evan, may you soar peacefully to new heights iii ACKNOWLEDGMENTS There are many people to whom I offer my gratitude for their assistance in completion of this project. I am especially grateful for the patience, suggestions, and advice provide by members of my committee: Dr. Frank Boster who is credited with the ideas contained within this project; Dr. Bradley Greenberg for insight into alternative hypotheses; Dr. Jim Dearing (who missed the proposal defense just because Sam went into labor with Spike) for his direction and encouragement; and to Dr. Kim Witte whose pragmatic approach to the process made the project bearable. This research was supported in part by an appointment to the Research Participation Program at the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, National Institute for Occupational Safety and Health administered by the Oak Ridge Institute for Science Education through an interagency agreement between the US. Department of Energy and CDC. The author acknowledges Alan Janssen for access to the NIOSH resources and this opportunity; Dick Tardif; and Susan Jacques for her artwork. Thank you, Deb Tigner for the administrative support provided in “just doing your job”. To Ann Wooten, Marge Barkman, and the rest of the administrative staff over the course of my graduate career at MSU, a debt of gratitude is owed. In addition, I am grateful for the wisdom and counsel of Sandi Smith, Chuck Atkin, Ron Tamborini, Patrice Buzzanell, Steve Wilson and Leslie Snyder. It is reassuring to know that someone can see the light at the end of the tunnel when you cannot. Finally, thanks to my family for their support. And mice: look, I make it through the hoop! TABLE OF CONTENTS List of Tables ............................................................................................. vi List of Figures ........................................................................................... viii Chapter 1. Introduction ............................................................................. 1 Chapter 2. Literature Review .................................................................... 5 Chapter 3. Method .................................................................................... 33 Chapter 4. Results .................................................................................... 47 Chapter 5. Summary and Conclusions ....................................................... 61 List of References ...................................................................................... 73 LIST OF TABLES Table 1. Items used to measure Sensation Seeking factors based on F orrn V of Zuckerman’s Sensation Seeking Scale (1979) .............. 42 Table 2. Variables measured in the study ..................................................... 45 Table 3. List of independent and dependent variables for the manipulation checks, hypotheses, and research questions ............................ 46 Table 4. Means, standard deviations, and cell counts for overall sensation seeking and each sensation seeking factor by message choice ....... 51 Table 5. Adjusted means, standard deviations, and cell counts for Attitude at Time 1 by message choice, message threat level, and sensation seeking ........................................................................................ 57 Table 6. Adjusted means, standard deviations, and cell counts for Intentions at Time 1 by message choice, message threat level, and sensation seeking ...................................................................................... 57 Table 7. Adjusted means, standard deviations, and cell counts for Behavior at Time 1 by message choice, message threat level, and sensation seeking ........................................................................................ 58 Table 8. Adjusted means, standard deviations, and cell counts for Attitudes at Time 2 by message choice, message threat level, and sensation seeking ........................................................................................ 58 Table 9. Adjusted means, standard deviations, and cell counts for Intentions at Time 2 by message choice, message threat level, and sensation seeking ........................................................................................ 58 Table 10. Adjusted means, standard deviations, and cell counts for Behaviors at Time 2 by message choice, message threat level, and sensation seeking .................................................................................. 59 Table l 1. Adjusted means, standard deviations, and cell counts for “Flossing more often” by message choice, message threat level, and sensation seeking ........................................................................................ 59 Table 12. Adjusted means, standard deviations, and cell counts for “Flossing more carefiilly” by message choice, message threat level, and sensation seeking .................................................................................. 59 Table 13. Adjusted means, standard deviations, and cell counts for “Flossing more regularly” by message choice, message threat level, and sensation seeking .................................................................................. 6O vii LIST OF FIGURES Figure 1. The Extended Parallel Process Model (EPPM) (Witte, 1992b) 7 Figure 2. Main effects predicted for Sensation Seeking and Message Threat Level ................................................................................. 26 viii CHAPTER 1 INTRODUCTION In order to promote health protective behaviors, campaigners commonly attempt to arouse fear in audiences. Indeed, Freimuth, Hammond, Edgar, and Monahan (1990) found that 26 percent of all AIDS public service announcements utilized fear appeals to promote health protective behavior. Fear appeals are messages designed to arouse fear in the receiver by describing the terrible consequences associated with non-compliance with the message recommendations (O’Keefe, 1990). It is presumed that these messages instill fear in the audience and spur members to modify their behaviors and to undertake the recommended beneficial health behaviors. In general, fear appeals have been found to be an effective means of gaining compliance with a message’s recommendations. Meta-analytic results indicate that a positive relationship exists between fear level and attitudes, intentions, and behaviors, such that strong fear appeals are more persuasive than weak fear appeals (Boster & Mongeau, 1984; Hale & Dillard, 1995; Sutton, 1982; Witte & Allen, 1996). Thus, we would expect messages which instill greater fear arousal would result in more behavior change. A number of models have arisen which suggest reasons why fear appeals are successful in gaining compliance. One model in particular, the Extended Parallel Process Model (EPPM) (W itte, 1992b), suggests the conditions under which risk messages are likely to be accepted and result in health protective behaviors. The EPPM posits that when individuals believe themselves to be at-risk for a serious health threat and believe that they can easily and effectively avert the threat, they change their attitudes, intentions, and behaviors to conform with the position advocated. However, if individuals do not believe that the recommended response will be effective or they believe that they are unable to perform it, then they are motivated to take action to reduce their fear. Although the EPPM explains when a fear appeal message is likely to be accepted or rejected, it has two shortcomings. One shortcoming of the model is the lack of attention given to individual difference variables. Although the EPPM suggests that individual differences indirectly influence the likelihood of engaging in health protective action through perceptions of threat and efficacy, it does not specify the manner in which it occurs. One variable in particular, sensation seeking, has been shown to influence reactions to fear appeals in unexpected ways and is in need of further study. Second, the fear appeal literature fails to take into account that people selectively expose themselves to information. In reality, people seek out and pay attention to information which is consistent with their beliefs and which does not exceed their desired arousal level; however, the vast majority of fear appeal studies force exposure to high or low threat fear appeal messages. These issues will be addressed in the study. One health concern which has been the focus in persuasion research is dental hygiene. Proper dental care has been used in tests of inoculation theory (e. g., McGuire, 1961; McGuire, 1962; McGuire & Papageorgis, 1961), theory of reasoned action and theory of planned behavior (e.g., McCaul, O’Neill, & Glasgow, 1988; Mch1, Sandgren, O’Neill, & Hinsz, 1993), and fear appeal research (e. g., Beck & Lund, 1981; Dembrowski, Lasater, & Ramirez, 1978; Evans, Rozelle, Noblitt, & Williams, 1975; Janis & Feshbach, 1953; Janis & Feshbach, 1954). Proper dental hygiene (e.g., a diligent regimen of brushing and flossing) aids in the prevention of periodontal (gum) disease. A proper regimen will remove the bacteria which cause plaque and can lead to periodontal disease. Without proper removal, bacteria-laden plaque builds up and may cause infections in the natural v-shaped gaps (called the sulcus) between teeth and the gum line. Untreated, the depth of the sulcus increases, allowing the gum line to recede from the teeth. Additional symptoms of early stages of gum disease include redness, swelling and tenderness of the gums, and bleeding when pressure is applied (Carranza, 1990; Cross, 1977). It is estimated that up to 75 percent of us have bacterial infections associated with periodontal disease (Schlosberg, 1996). When treated early, most cases of periodontal disease are reversible; however, diligent dental hygiene is necessary to prevent the onset or recurrence of periodontal disease (Carranza, 1990; Schlosberg, 1996). Although most people know the importance of proper dental hygiene, many do not floss on a regular basis (Camner, Sandell, Sarhed, 1994). The development and evaluation of fear appeals intended to induce behavioral compliance with dental hygiene regimens will benefit from the incorporation of knowledge based on current theoretical work and research findings regarding fear appeals. In sum, the purpose of this thesis is to extend the EPPM to incorporate the influences of sensation seeking, selective exposure, and message choice in order to prevent periodontal disease. Integrating these factors into the EPPM should increase the predictive utility of the model and provide a stronger understanding of the influence of sensation seeking on the desire to engage in health protective action. The next chapter describes the EPPM, selective exposure, sensation seeking, and expectancy theory and offers hypotheses. Chapter 3 presents the methodology for the experiment and describes the health domain for the study. Chapter 4 presents results of the analyses. Finally, Chapter 5 includes discussion and implications for the theory and for practitioners. CHAPTER 2 LITERATURE REVIEW Instilling fear in an audience in order to promote behavior change is a technique which is commonly used (Atkin, 1994; Dillard, 1994; O’Keefe, 1990). The use of fear has been the focus of numerous research projects in order to gain a greater understanding of when and why fear appeals are effective in gaining attitude or behavior change (Boster & Mongeau, 1984; Dillard, 1994; O’Keefe, 1990; Witte, 1992b). Fear appeals are persuasive messages which are designed to fiighten the audience through the use of vivid, gruesome language regarding the consequences of not performing the advocated behaviors (Boster & Mongeau, 1984; O’Keefe, 1990; Witte, 1992b). Meta-analyses have demonstrated that fear appeals have a small but consistent impact on behavior (Boster & Mongeau, 1984; Mongeau, 1994; Witte & Allen, 1996). In their meta-analysis, Witte and Allen (1996) found that stronger fear appeals were consistently more persuasive than weaker fear appeals. Stronger fear appeals are characterized by their ability to create greater perceptions of the severity of the threat and one’s susceptibility to it. Boster and Mongeau (1984) noted that the size of the correlation between fear manipulations and perceived fear was relatively small (r = .36), suggesting that experimental inductions have been weak. This finding suggests that larger discrepancies between high threat and low threat fear appeals would increase the correlation between fear manipulations and perceived fear. The creation of a stronger fear appeal, theoretically, would instill greater perceptions of severity and susceptibility. 6 In addition, Witte and Allen (1996) noted that the messages which were most successful in promoting message acceptance were stronger fear appeals which contained stronger efficacy messages. Thus, the success of a fear appeal is linked to its ability to create perceptions that the individual may easily and effectively avert a severe threat despite one’s susceptibility to it. Recently, in an attempt to reconcile inconsistencies within the fear appeal literature, Witte (1992b) proposed a model which attempted to explain when and why fear appeals are likely to succeed in promoting attitude and behavior change. This chapter begins with a discussion of a recent fear appeal theory, the Extended Parallel Process Model (W itte, 1992b). Next, the EPPM’s shortcomings are presented and discussed. These shortcomings are addressed in reviews of selective exposure and sensation seeking. Finally, hypotheses are offered. The Extended Pa_rall_el Process Model Witte (1992b) developed the Extended Parallel Process Model (EPPM) in order to detail more explicitly when and why fear appeals are effective in gaining attitude and behavior change. As an extension of Leventhal’s (1970) parallel process model, the EPPM differentiates between two responses to a fear appeal message: fear control and danger control (see Figure 1). The EPPM suggests that individuals exposed to external stimuli (e.g., a fear appeal message) will make judgments on the threat of the hazard and the efficacy of the recommended response. Threat motivates action; therefore, if people do not believe themselves to be at-risk to a severe threat, no further processing of the message occurs. Thus, threat appraisal determines whether or not individuals will respond $32 .256 95%: .082 £85 ease 322$ 2: ._ 2:5 80:08th .3222: A383. ozv “33.5 8:83.852 =o_.uo_.um =o=§€2 ~ at .30 causes. Al 2:885 S: J .3» . 26m k $232838 begun a... .2me 5:32:33 “.833. Qm>~mummm auauEm umzoamum I: II l. l ..l .l. I. II I. «fin All I IV A. 335923 Quaowmm 8:33”. . . .aoaumwmcfimv memZOAIOU 233k >U<0Em mo .10). Messa es A single message was varied across levels for this study. Although multiple message designs are preferred to increase generalizability, a single message design was chosen because the main purpose of this study is to examine relations among theoretical variables (e. g., sensation seeking, threat, and efficacy). A high threat and low threat message were developed by compiling verbatim passages from several periodontal disease textbooks, brochures, and other sources (Beck & Lund, 1981; Bhaskar, 1986; Carranza, 1990; Cross, 1977; Dembrowski, Lasater, & Ramirez, 1978; Lamey & Lewis, 1988; Schlosberg, 1996; Wilson, 1989; Wilson, & Komman, 1996). The messages were validated in a separate study described in the next section. Efficacy was held high at a constant level (M = 5.78, sd = .70, N = 83 for response efficacy and M = 5.26, sd = .87, N 36 = 82 for self-efficacy on a 7-point Likert type scale). Following the format used by Beck and Lund (1981) and Dembrowski, Lasater, and Ramirez (197 8), the messages stated the cause of periodontal disease; the areas of the gum affected by it; the progression of the disease; its eventual outcome; and a recommended response (brushing and flossing) to avoid its onset. The high threat message was developed based on Witte’s (1993) guidelines and focused on (a) individual’s susceptibility to periodontal disease and (b) the severity of the disease. Additionally, the high threat message contained vivid, personalistic, and graphic language. All of the information in the message was accurate. Each message was accompanied by four pictures obtained from periodontal textbooks. Pictures accompanying the low threat message depicted healthy gums, plaque build-up, and a mild case of gingivitis (an early stage of periodontal disease). Pictures accompanying the high threat message depicted plaque stained with blue food dye, chronic inflammatory gingival enlargements, gingival recession, and soft-tissue swelling. Message Validation Study To ensure the messages differed in the degree of threat and fear they produce, the fear appeal messages were subjected to pilot-testing and validation in accordance with procedures found in the fear appeal literature. A separate study was undertaken to validate the high threat message in fact acted as a fear appeal to produce stronger threat perceptions and fear when compared to the low-threat message. Eighty-three students were recruited from a mid-sized university in southern New England and were randomly assigned to read versions of the high threat or low threat message. Participants were asked to carefirlly read their message and then complete a 64 item questionnaire that 37 determined perceptions of threat (i.e., severity and susceptibility), efficacy (i.e., response efficacy and self-efficacy) and fear. Specifically, 7-point Likert-type scales (ranging fi'om “strongly disagree” to “strongly agree”) were used to assess: (a) sci/m (harmfirl; bad for my health; have negative consequences; dangerous; serious; severe); (b) susceptibilig (likely; possible; at-risk; probably); (c) response efficag (works; effective; adequately prevents; reduce my chances; worthwhile); (d) self-efficacy (able to floss; easy to do; convenient; does not take too much effort; not difficult; simple); and (e) fe_ar (uncomfortable; frightens; scares; anxious; nervous; tense). The validation study results indicated that the high threat message produced significantly stronger severity and susceptibility perceptions and significantly greater fear than the low threat message. After confirmatory factor analyses, items for each factor were combined to form an index for that factor. The high threat message produced significantly more severity perceptions (M = 6.22, sd = .73, N = 40 on a 7 point Likert- type scale) than the low threat message (M = 4.79, sd = 1.06, N = 43 on a 7 point Likert- type scale), t = -7.11, df= 81, p < .001 (g = .61, p < .001, N = 81). The high threat message also produced significantly more susceptibility perceptions (M = 4.63, sd = 1.37, 1_\l_ = 40, on a 7 point Likert-type scale) than the low threat message (M = 3.02, sd = 1.10, N = 43, on a 7 point Likert-type scale), t = -5.92, df= 81, p < .001 (g = .56, p < .001, N 81). Similarly, the high threat message aroused significantly greater levels of fear (M = 5.11, sd = 1.15, N = 39, on a 7 point Likert-type scale) than the low threat message (M = 4.07, sd = 1.38, N = 43, on a 7 point Likert-type scale), t = -3.69, df= 80, p < .001 (I = .38, p < .001, N = 81). 38 Both high and low threat messages were compared to versions without the efficacy component. No significant differences were revealed for perceptions of response efficacy (E (3, 79) = .45, p = ns) or for self-efficacy (E (3, 78) = 1.26, p = ns). Overall, efficacy perceptions were high (M = 5.78, sd = .70, 15 = 83 for response efficacy and M = 5.26, sd = .87, N = 82 for self-efficacy on 7-point Likert type scales). Measures All scales were measured using 7 point Likert-type scales unless otherwise noted. All measures were developed based on Ajzen and F ishbein’s (1977) guidelines for measures to correspond in specificity. Scales were developed using the confirmatory factor analysis subroutine of Hunter’s (1988) Package. Confirmatory factor analyses (Hunter & Gerbing, 1982) were employed to test the dimensionality of the measures. At the end of this section, two tables have been included. Table 2 summarizes the variables measured in the study. Table 3 summarizes the independent and dependent variables for the manipulation checks, hypotheses, and research questions. Threat The two components of threat, severity and susceptibility were measured with five items each. Perceived severity of periodontal disease was assessed with such questions as: (a) “I believe periodontal disease is bad for my health”; (b) “I believe periodontal disease has negative consequences”; (c) “I believe that periodontal disease is dangerous”; ((1) “I believe periodontal disease is serious”; and (e) “I believe periodontal disease is severe”. After confirmatory factor analysis were performed, the scale items were summed and 39 averaged to create an overall index for perceived severity. Cronbach’s alpha for perceived severity at Time 1 was .88 and at Time 2 was .94. Perceived susceptibility was assessed with such questions as: (a) “I am likely to get periodontal disease”; (b) “It is possible that I will get periodontal disease”; (c) “I am at-risk for periodontal disease”; ((1) “I probably will get periodontal disease”; and (e) Chances are I will get periodontal disease”. The scale items were summed averaged to create an overall index for perceived susceptibility. Cronbach’s alpha for perceived susceptibility at Time 1 was .91 and at Time 2 was .92. M! The two components of efficacy, response efficacy and self-efiicacy were assessed with four and six items, respectively. Perceived response efficacy was assessed with such questions as: (a) “I believe that flossing on a regular basis works in preventing periodontal disease”; (1)) “I believe that flossing on a regular basis will be effective in preventing periodontal disease”; (c) “I believe that flossing on a regular basis will adequately prevent periodontal disease”; and (d) “I believe that flossing on a regular basis will reduce my chances of getting periodontal disease”. The scale items were summed averaged to create an overall index for perceived response efficacy. Cronbach’s alpha for perceived response efficacy at Time 1 was .78 and at Time 2 was .87. Perceived self-efficacy was assessed with such questions as: (a) “I am able to floss on a regular basis to reduce my chances of getting periodontal disease”; (13) “Flossing on a regular basis to prevent periodontal disease is easy to do”; (c) “Flossing on a regular basis to prevent periodontal disease is convenient”; (d) “Flossing on a regular basis to prevent 4o periodontal disease does mt take too much effort”; (e) “Flossing on a regular basis to prevent periodontal disease is n_ot difficult”; and (f) “Flossing on a regular basis to prevent periodontal disease is simple”. The scale items were summed and averaged to create an overall index for perceived response efiicacy. Cronbach’s alpha for perceived self-efficacy at Time 1 was .87 and at Time 2 was .90 Pia: Fear arousal was measured by having participants rate the following mood adjectives (“strongly disagree” to “strongly agree”): anxious; nervous; scares; fiightens; and uncomfortable. The scale items were summed and averaged to create an overall index for perceived fear. Cronbach’s alpha for fear at Time 1 was .87 and at Time 2 was .91. Danger control outcomes Attitude. Attitudes toward flossing on a regular basis were assessed with three seven-point semantic differential scales (e. g., good/bad, beneficial/not beneficial, and favorable/unfavorable). Cronbach’s alpha for these items at Time 1 was .68 and at Time 2 was .73. Intentions. Intentions to floss on a regular basis were measured with seven questions on a seven-point Likert type scale. These questions included: (a) “I plan to floss on a regular basis to prevent periodontal disease”; (b) “I intend to floss on a regular basis to prevent periodontal disease”; (c) “I am going to floss on a regular basis to prevent periodontal disease”; (d) “I expect to floss on a regular basis to prevent periodontal disease”; (e) “I intend to buy dental floss”; and (f) “I intend to use dental floss 41 on a regular basis”. Cronbach’s alpha for intentions at Time 1 was .97 and at Time 2 was .96. Behaviors. Current behavioral practices were assessed for everyone during the initial interview. Participants were asked how often they flossed their teeth. In addition, self-report measures of behavioral performance were asked. These questions included: (a) “I currently floss on a regular basis”; (b) “I consistently floss on a regular basis”; and (c) “I regularly floss”. Cronbach’s alpha for this index at Time 1 was .97 and at Time 2 was .98. Fear control measures Defenaive Avoidance. Participants will be asked to respond to the following question in four ways, “When I first heard about periodontal disease, my first instinct was to”: (a) “Want to think about/not want to think about my health risk”; (b) “Want/Not want to protect myself from periodontal disease”; (c) “Think about it a lot/Put problem out of my mind”; and ((1) “Think about the problem/Try to forget the problem”. Cronbach’s alpha at Time 1 was .85 and at Time 2 was .88. Perceived Manipulation. The perceived manipulation questions asked if the participants felt that the message was: manipulative, misleading, or distorted. Cronbach’s alpha at Time 1 was .82. Issue Derogation. The issue derogation questions asked whether the participants thought the reports of periodontal disease were: overblown, overstated, exaggerated, or overestimated. Cronbach’s alpha at Time 1 was .97 and at Time 2 was .96. Sensation seekipg 42 Sensation seeking was measured using Form V of Zuckerman’s Sensation Seeking Scale Form V (1979). This instrument includes forty forced choice questions. Zuckerman’s measurement scale was chosen based on its extensive validation and the replication of its factors across a variety of topics with multiple populations. Confirmatory factor analyses were performed in order to distinguish among the four factors (i.e., Thrill and Adventure Seeking; Experience Seeking; Disinhibition; and Boredom Susceptibility). Table 1 shows the items which met the criteria for internal consistency and parallelism (Hunter & Gerbing, 1982). In addition, the four sub-factors were analyzed in order to determine if they were second-order unidimensional according to the procedures outlined by Hunter and Gerbing (1982). The four factors failed to meet the criteria for second order unidimensionality; thus, analyses were conducted for the factors only. In addition to correlational tests, subsequent analyses participants in the upper tertile of each factor were classified as high sensation seekers for that dimension while those in the lower tertile were classified as low sensation seekers for that dimension. Table 1. Items used to measure Sensation Seeking factors based on Form V of Zuckerman’s Sensation Seeking Scale (1979) Factor No. Items mtg Thrill and Adventure Seeking 10 3A 11B 16A 17A 20B 213 23A 28A 38B 408 Experience Seeking 5 48 6A 19B 22A 26B Disinhibition 7 1A 13B 29B 303 33B 35B 36A Boredom Susceptibility 6 5A 7B 8A 31B 34A 39A Cronbach’s alpha for the four factors were as follows: Thrill and Adventure Seeking, a = .81; Experience Seeking or = .36; Disinhibition or = .71; and Boredom Susceptibility or = 43 .54. Given the low reliabilities for experience seeking and boredom susceptibility, caution should be used in interpreting the results from tests with those factors. The reduced reliability indicates a potential lack of consistency and stability in the measures. It should be noted however, that the reliability obtained for boredom susceptibility is approximately that obtained in other studies which used this scale (e. g., Zuckennan, Eysenck, & Eysenck, 1978; Zuckerrnan, 1971). Theahold questions Because it is possible for the high threat fear appeal to exceed the optimal level of arousal of participants, five questions were asked to detemrine if the threshold was exceeded. Questions using a seven point Likert-type scale included: (a) “This poster is too scary; it should be milder”; (b) “This poster was too mild; it should be scarier”; (c) “This poster was not as scary as I thought it would be”; ((1) “This poster was too extreme; it should be toned down”; and (e) “This poster was too serious; it should be milder”. Confound Checks Participants’ beliefs about and their perceived comprehension of the message were assessed. Participants responded to the question “This message was. . . on a seven point Likert-type scale: boring, exciting, interesting, offensive, and easy to understand. In addition, participants will be asked to evaluate how much they learned fi'om reading the message (e. g., “I learned a lot from reading this message”). Finally, respondents will evaluate the quality of the arguments presented on a seven-point semantic differential scale (e.g., strong/weak; outstanding/poor; superior/inferior; great/lousy; marvelous/terrible; impressive/unimpressive). 44 Demographic Variables Sex, age, ethnicity, current flossing behavior, current brushing behavior, smoking, family history of diabetes, and history of periodontal disease were assessed for use as control variables when needed. 45 Table 2. Variables measured in the study Extended Parallel Process Measures Perceived threat: perceived severity and perceived susceptibility Perceived efficacy: perceived response efficacy and perceived self-efiicacy F ear Danger control: attitudes, intentions, and behaviors Fear control: defensive avoidance, issue derogation, perceived manipulation Sensation Seeking (Zuckerman’s Form V) Thrill and adventure seeking Experience seeking Disinhibition Boredom susceptibility Message choice: Choice: traditional “forced” exposure or allowed to choose (high threat or low threat message) For those allowed to choose, received choice or received the message they did not choose Threshold questions Confound checks Demographic variables 46 Table 3. List of independent and dependent variables for the manipulation checks, hypotheses, and research questions. Manipulation Check, Hypothesis or Independent Dependent Research Question Variable(s) Variable(s) Manipulation check Message threat level Perceived severity, (high or low) perceived susceptibility, Manipulation check Hypothesis 1 Research Question 1 Hypothesis 2 Hypothesis 3 Research question 2 Hypothesis 4 Hypothesis 5 Message efficacy level Sensation Seeking (overall) Sensation Seeking (by factor) Sensation seeking (overall and by factor) Message threat level (high or low) Sensation seeking (by factor) Message choice (received choice versus received message rejected) Message choice, Sensation Seeking, and Message threat level perceived fear Perceived response efficacy, perceived self-efficacy Message choice (high threat or low threat) Message choice (high threat or low threat) Danger control outcomes (attitudes, intentions, behaviors) Danger control outcomes (attitudes, intentions, behaviors) Danger control outcomes (attitudes, intentions, and behaviors) Danger control outcomes (attitudes, intentions, and behaviors) Danger control outcomes (attitudes, intentions, and behaviors) CHAPTER 4 RESULTS 9m The purpose of this study was to examine the effect of sensation seeking and message choice on responses to fear appeals. Results of the manipulation and confound checks are presented followed by results for the hypotheses presented in Chapter 2. A complete description of the message validation study procedures and results appear in Chapter 3. Manipulation Checks -- High versus Low Threat Message Manipulation checks were conducted using one-tailed t-tests to determine if the high threat message produced stronger perceptions of threat and fear than the low threat message. In addition, two-tailed t-tests were employed to ensure that efiicacy was held constant across both messages. To ensure that no self-selection features were factored into the manipulation checks, the analyses were performed with the 75 individuals who were randomly assigned to the forced exposure condition (i.e., they were not asked to choose between the two fear appeal messages). Perceived threat. The high threat message produced significantly more severity perceptions (M = 6.05, sd = .78, N = 38, on a 7 point Likert-type scale) than the low threat message (M = 5.48, sd = .85, N = 37, on a 7 point Likert-type scale), t = 3.04, df = 73, p < .05 (r = .34, p < .01, N = 75). The high threat message produced significantly greater perceptions of susceptibility (.M. = 3.45, sd = 1.14, N = 38, on a 7 point Likert-type scale) than the low threat message (M = 2.96, sd = 1.23, N = 37, on a 7 point Likert-type scale), t = 1.83, df= 72, p < .05 (g = .21, p < .05, a = 74). 47 48 Fear. The high threat message produced significantly greater fear (M = 5.47, sd = 1.20, N = 38, on a 7 point Likert-type scale) than the low threat message M = 4.95, sd = 1.33, N = 37, on a 7 point Likert-type scale), t = 1.79, df= 73, p < .05 (g = .20, p < .05, N = 7 5). Perceived efficaey. No significant differences were revealed for perceptions of response efficacy (t = -.87, df = 72, p = ns) or for self-efficacy (t = -l .25, df = 72 p = ns). Overall, efficacy perceptions were high (M = 5.81, sd = .73, N = 74 for response efficacy and M = 5.47, sd = .97, N = 74 for self-efficacy on 7-point Likert type scales). Confound Chealas. No significant differences were found between the high and low threat messages on their descriptive qualities (e. g., excitement, interest, etc.), the amount of information learned from the message, or argument quality. Manipulation Checks -- Choice Conditions Additional analyses were conducted to rule out any effects choice versus no choice might have had on threat perceptions for the high versus low threat messages. For example, we were interested in assessing whether or not the high threat message produced the same degree of severity, susceptibility and fear for those in the choice vs. no choice conditions. No significant differences emerged between the two groups of individuals who read the high threat message only (choice versus no choice) on perceived severity (t = -. 12, df = 88, p = ns), perceived susceptibility (t = .69, (if = 88, p = ns), fear (t = .36, df = 87, p = ns), perceived response efficacy (t = -1.89, df = 88, p = us), or perceived self- effrcacy (t = .96, df = 88, p = ns). Similarly, no significant differences emerged between the two groups of individuals who read the low threat message only (choice versus no 49 choice) on perceived severity (t = .41, (if = 89, p = ns), perceived susceptibility (t = -l.87, df = 89, p = ns), fear (t = -1.24, df = 89, p = ns), perceived response efficacy (t = -.79, df = 89, p = ns), or perceived self-efficacy (t = -1.42, df = 89, p = ns). Sensation Seeking and Message Choice Hypothesis 1, that the likelihood of choosing a high threat message will be positively related to sensation seeking level, was tested using correlational analyses. This technique is appropriate when assessing the relationship between an intervally scaled independent variable and a dichotomous dependent variable (Watt & van den Berg, 1995). Similar to other sensation seeking studies, analyses using the upper and lower tertiles of sensation seeking were also performed (e. g., Clayton, Cattarello, & Walden, 1991; Palmgreen, Donohew, Lorch, Rogus, Helm & Grant, 1991; Witte & Morrison, 1995). Hypothesis 1 was not supported. There was no relationship between sensation seeking and message choice whether a total sensation seeking score was used (1; = -.09, N = 177, p = ns) or if the efiect of all four factors was considered independently (R = .12, N = 177, p = ns). In total, 34.5% of all subjects chose the high threat fear appeal while 64.5% chose the low threat message. Analyses using only the upper and lower tertiles (X2 (l, N = 139) = .15, p = ns) yielded similar results.2 Research Question I asked if there was any relationship between the individual sensation seeking factors’ levels and the likelihood of choosing a high threat message. Table 4 summarizes the mean, standard deviation, and cell counts by message choice (high 2The break point for sensation seeking tertiles were calculated using the entire sample, regardless of condition. Respondents whose scores fell on the cut off point for the upper and lower tertile were classified in that group. 50 or low threat). Individuals high in thrill and adventure seeking were not more likely to choose the high threat fear appeal than were individuals low in thrill and adventure seeking (g = -.04, N = 177, p = ns). Analyses using only the upper and lower tertiles (X2 (1, N = 106) = .03, p = ns) yielded similar results. Individuals high in boredom susceptibility were not more likely to choose the high threat fear appeal than were individuals low in boredom susceptibility. Although no overall relationship was found, (r = -.07, N = 177, p = ns), analyses using only the upper and lower tertiles revealed that those highest in the upper tertile for boredom susceptibility were more likely to choose the message low threat message (“designed to arouse curiosity”) than the high threat message (“explicit and scary”) (X2 (1, N = 141) = 4.70, p < .05). Experience seeking and disinhibition were also unrelated to a person’s likelihood of choosing a high threat fear appeal was supported. No relationship was found between experience seeking and message choice (r = .02, N = 177, p = ns) or disinhibition and message choice (1 = -. l 1, N = 177, p = ns). Analyses using only the upper and lower tertiles for experience seeking (X2 (1, N = 119) = .19, p = ns) and for disinhibition (X2 (1, N = 136) = 2.19, p = ns) yielded similar results. 51 Table 4. Means, standard deviations, and cell counts for overall Sensation Seeking level and each sensation seeking factor by Message Choice. Chose High Threat Chose Low Threat Message Message Thrill and Adventure M = 6.78 out of 10* M = 6.94 out of 10* t = .38 Seeking sd = 2.93 sd = 2.55 df= 180" N = 64 N = 118 p = ns Experience Seeking M = 2.16 out of 5* M = 2.11 out of 5* t = -.21 sd= 1.00 sd= 1.20 df=179** N = 64 N = 117 p = ns Disinhibition M = 3.06 out of 7* M = 3.58 out of 7* t = 1.63 sd=2.12 sd=2.06 df=181** N = 65 N = 118 p = ns Boredom M = 1.74 out of 6* M = 2.03 out of 6* t = 1.29 Susceptibility sd = 1.44 sd = 1.49 df = 184" N = 66 N = 120 p = ns Overall Sensation M = 13.87 out of28* M = 14.76 out of28* t = 1.21 Seeking sd = 5.02 sd = 4.44 df= 175" N = 62 N = 115 p = ns *Number of items per scale "Cell counts differ by factor due to missing data Sensation Seeflag, Attitudes. Intentions, ml Behaviors Hypothesis 2 posited that sensation seeking would be negatively related to attitudes, intentions, and behaviors toward engaging in health protective action. Specifically, high sensation seekers would have more positive attitudes, intentions, and behaviors toward engaging in the risky behavior and, conversely, that low sensation seekers would have more positive attitudes, intentions and behaviors towards performing the recommended response of flossing. Research Question 2 sought to determine how each of the sensation seeking factors were related to attitudes, intentions, and behaviors. To test this hypothesis research question, correlation analyses were performed. Most participants at the time of the initial interview had positive attitudes towards flossing (N = 6.28, sd = .83, N = 256). The overall sensation seeking factor was unrelated to attitudes 52 at the time of the initial interview or at the two week follow-up. None of the four sensation seeking factors was related to attitudes at Time 1. Only individuals low in boredom susceptibility had more positive attitudes toward engaging in the recommended response at Time 2 (r = -. 14, N = 209, p < .05). Intentions at Tirnel were negatively related to overall sensation seeking (r = -.10, N = 23 7, p < .05), though they were unrelated at Time 2. Individuals low in disinhibition and individuals low in boredom susceptibility expressed greater intention to floss on a regular basis initially (I = -. 13, N = 246, p < .05, and r = -. 16, N = 251, p < .05, respectively). The overall sensation seeking factor was unrelated to behaviors at Time 1 and Time 2. However, individuals low in sensation seeking reported brushing more regularly (r = -.14, N = 201, p < .05), brushing more carefully (I = -. 18, N = 201, p < .01), brushing longer (1 = -.16, N = 201, p < .01), brushing more often (1 = -.19, N = 201, p < .01), as well as talking with someone else about brushing one’s teeth (r; = -.14, N = 200, p < .05). Individuals low in disinhibition also reported initially regular flossing (r = -.11, N = 247, p < .05). Individuals low in boredom susceptibility also reported flossing more carefully (; = - .14, N = 211, p < .05), brushing more often (_r_ = -.13, N = 210, p < .05), and talking with someone else about brushing (I = -.16, N = 210, p < .05) at Time 2. In addition, those low in thrill and adventure seeking reported that since participating in the study, they brushed their teeth more regularly (r; = -.16, N = 206, p < .05), more carefully, (r; = -.23, N = 205, p < .05), more often (r = -.21, N = 205, p < .05), and for longer periods of time (_r_ = -.19, N = 206, p < .05) as well as talking with someone else about brushing one’s teeth (1* = -.13, N = 205, p < .05). Thus, Hypothesis 2 received partial support; though few 53 significant differences emerged, intentions and behaviors were negatively related to sensation seeking level. Message Threat Level. Attitudes. Intentions. a_nd Behaviors Hypothesis 3 posited that the high threat message would produce more positive attitudes, intentions, and behaviors regardless of sensation seeking level. This hypothesis was assessed using an independent sample t-test. Although the high threat message did not produce more positive attitudes and intentions either at the immediate post test or at the two-week follow-up, behaviors were affected. Specifically, individuals who read the high threat message reported flossing more often (t = -2.22, df = 210, p <05); flossing more carefirlly (t = -3.10, df = 210, p <05); flossing more regularly (t = -2.S6, df = 209, p <05); brushing more regularly (t = -2.52, df = 210, p <05); brushing more carefully (t = - 2.85, df= 210, p <05); brushing for longer periods of time (t = -3.59, df = 210, p <05); and brushing more often (t = -2.58, df = 209, p <05). Thus, Hypothesis 3 was supported for behaviors, but not for attitudes or intentions. Choice. Attitudeafltentionwd Behaviors Hypothesis 4, that individuals receiving the message they chose would have more positive attitudes, intentions, and behaviors than those who received the message they rejected regardless of sensation seeking level, was partially supported. Although no differences emerged for attitudes or intentions, behaviors were affected. Individuals who received the message they rejected reported flossing their teeth more often (t = -2.50, df = 146, p < .05); flossing more carefully (t = -2.03, df = 146, p <05); flossing more regularly (t = -2.60, df= 145, p <05) and brushing longer (t = -1.94, df= 210, p <05). Moreover, 54 those individuals who chose the low threat but received the high threat message reported flossing more often (t = -2.64, df = 74, p < .01) and flossing more regularly (t = -2.57, df = 74, p < .01) than those who chose and received the high threat message. C_ho_iea Post-hoc sub-analyses were conducted on the choice-only condition to determine the perceptions of those who might choose the high threat message versus the low threat message, as it was thought these perceptions might be related to attitudes, intentions, and behaviors. Of the 186 participants given the choice of which message they preferred to read, 66 chose the high threat message (35.5%) while 120 chose the low threat message (64.5%). Interestingly, analyses revealed that individuals who chose the high threat message, regardless of message received, had significantly greater perceived susceptibility (M = 3.32, sd = 1.15, N = 66, on a 7 point Likert-type scale) than those who chose the low threat message (M = 3.00, sd = 1.24, N = 120, on a 7 point Likert-type scale), t = -1.75, df=179, p <05. Choice, Message Threat Level. Swtion Seeking and Attitudes, Intentions and Behaa/iors Hypothesis 5 predicted main effects for sensation seeking, message choice, and message threat level for attitudes, intentions, and behaviors. Tables 5 to 13 show the adjusted means, standard deviations, and cell sizes for attitudes, intentions, and behaviors at Time 1 and Time 2 by message threat level, message choice, and sensation seeking level. Only subjects who were not in the forced exposure conditions were included in the following analyses.’ First, analysis of variance was performed in order to determine if there were any interaction effects among the factors and attitudes, intentions, and flossing 3 Degrees of freedom differ due to missing data. No variable had more than five percent of the data missing. 55 behaviors. No interactions were revealed. Thus, sensation seeking was treated as a covariate and analysis of covariance was performed. Analyses revealed no significant effects for the sensation seeking factors (E (4, 164) = .53, p = ns), message choice, (E (1, 164) = .51, p = ns), or message threat level (E (1, 164) = .15, p = ns) on attitudes at the immediate post-test (E (7, 164) = .53, p = ns). Similarly at the two-week follow-up, no significant effects on attitudes emerged (E (7, 132) = .94, p = ns). Analyses revealed no significant effects for the sensation seeking factors (E (4, 132) = 1.29, p = ns), message choice, (E (1, 132) = .70, p = ns), or message threat level (E (l, 132) = .18, p = ns). Thus, Hypothesis 5 was not supported for attitudes. With respect to intentions at the immediate post-test (E (7, 160) = 2.12, p < .05), no effects for message choice (E (1, 160) = .58, p = ns) or message threat level (E (1, 160) = .94, p = ns) emerged. Although none of the sensation seeking factors had a significant effect on intentions individually, they did have a significant effect on intentions when combined (E (4, 160) = 2.84, p < . 05). At the two-week follow-up, no significant effects for the sensation seeking factors (E (4, 133) = 1.00, p = ns), message choice, (E (1, 133) = 1.25, p = ns), or message threat level (E (1, 133) = .21, p = ns) emerged. Overall, at the two-week follow-up, there was no effect on intentions (E (7, 133) = .87, p = ns). Again, Hypothesis 5 was not supported for intentions. With respect to current behavioral practices at the immediate post-test, no significant effects emerged for either message choice (E (1, 161) = .13, p = ns) or message threat level (E (1, 161) = .22, p = ns). However, significant effects emerged for the 56 sensation seeking factor of disinhibition (E (1, 161) = 6.23, p < .05) and for the four factors in combination (E (1, 161) = 2.81, p < .05). At the two-week follow-up, no significant effects for the sensation seeking factors (E (4, 133) = 1.93, p = ns), message choice, (E (1, 133) = .81, p = ns), or message threat level (E (1, 133) = .03, p = ns) emerged. Overall, at the two-week follow-up, there was no effect on current behavioral practices (E (7, 133) = .87, p = ns). With respect to actual flossing behaviors at the two-week follow-up, although no significant effects emerged overall (E (7, 134) = 1.51, p = ns), individuals who received the message they rejected reported flossing more often than those individuals who received the message they chose (E (1, 134) = 5.32, p < .05). Yet, no differences emerged due to message threat level (E (1, 134) = .01, p = ns) or due to the sensation seeking factors (E (4, 134) = .55, p = ns). Similarly, although no significant effects emerged overall (E (7, 133) = 1.71, p = ns), individuals who received the message they rejected reported flossing more regularly than those individuals who received the message they chose (E (1, 133) = 5.16, p < .05). Again, no differences emerged due to message threat level (E (1, 133) = .20, p = ns) or due to the sensation seeking factors (E (4, 133) = .75, p = ns). However, individuals who received the message they rejected did not report flossing more carefully than those individuals who received the message they chose (E (1, 134) = 2.30, p = ns). Again, no differences emerged due to message threat level (E (1, 134) = 2.54, p = ns) or due to the sensation seeking factors (E (4, 134) = 1.28, p = ns), or for the model overall (E (7, 134) = 1.53, p = ns). Thus, Hypothesis 5 was not supported for behaviors. 57 Overall, the best predictor of behavior at the two-week follow-up was an individual’s flossing behavior at the immediate post-test ([ = .71, N = 206, p < .01). Those who flossed more frequently at the beginning of the study had more positive attitudes towards flossing (E = .22, N = 252, p < .01); more positive intentions to floss both at Time 1 (g = .41, N = 248, p < .01) and Time 2 (a = .34, N = 206, p < .01); and reported flossing more often (a = .17, N = 208, p < .05), more carefully (I = .19, N = 208, p <01), and more regularly (I = .24, N = 207, p < .01). Table 5. Means, standard deviations, and cell counts for Attitude at Time 1 by message choice, message threat level, and sensation seeking. Choice of Message No Choice of Message Low Sensation High Sensation Low Sensation High Sensation Seekers Seekers Seekers Seekers High Threat a = 6.39 a = 6.46 a = 6.14 a = 6.31 Message M = .88 M = .65 M = .83 M = .84 p =17 g = 16 p:20 a = 33 Low Threat a = 6.18 2; = 6.40 a = 6.27 a = 6.40 Message M= .72 M= .88 M= .51 M= .74 2:31 p=26 p=l4 __n=15 Table 6. Means, standard deviations, and cell counts for Intentions at Time 1 by message choice, message threat level, and sensation seeking. Choice of Message No Choice of Message Low Sensation High Sensation Low Sensation High Sensation Seekers Seekers Seekers Seekers High Threat a = 5.55 a = 5.46 a = 5.34 a = 5.51 Message M = .94 M = 1.07 M = 1.63 M = .87 p=17 p=16 p=l9 p=30 Low Threat a = 5.55 2; = 4.56 a = 5.68 a = 5.49 Message M = .77 M = 1.53 M = .78 d = 1.03 g=3l p=26 g=14 p=15 58 Table 7. Means, standard deviations, and cell counts for Behavior at Time 1 by message choice, message threat level, and sensation seeking. Choice of Message No Choice of Message Low Sensation High Sensation Low Sensation High Sensation Seekers Seekers Seekers Seekers High Threat a=4.38 a=401 _x=4.35 a=3.58 Message M= 1.70 M= 1.65 M= 1.91 M= 1.80 g=17 p=16 g=19 p=31 Low Threat a=4.41 a=306 a=4.4l a=3.64 Message M=1.98 M=201 M=1.77 M=1.95 g=31 p=26 p=14 p=15 Table 8. Means, standard deviations, and cell counts for Attitudes at Time 2 by message choice, message threat level, and sensation seeking. Choice of Message No Choice of Message Low Sensation High Sensation Low Sensation High Sensation Seekers Seekers Seekers Seekers High Threat a = 5.64 a = 6.05 a = 5.85 a = 6.25 Message M = .93 M = 1.07 M = .76 M = .82 p=l4 p=12 g=17 p=30 Low Threat a= 5.93 a=6.12 a= 5.91 a=6.28 Message M = .67 a_ = .94 M = .74 M = 1.21 p=27 g=22 p=10 g=8 Table 9. Means, standard deviations, and cell counts for Intentions at Time 2 by message choice, message threat level, and sensation seeking. Choice of Message No Choice of Message Low Sensation High Sensation Low Sensation High Sensation Seekers Seekers Seekers Seekers High Threat a = 5.07 a = 4.67 a = 5.13 2; = 4.80 Message d=l.26 M=1.05 M=1.35 M=1.11 g=14 p=12 p=l7 p=30 Low Threat a = 4.83 a = 4.47 a = 5.62 a = 4.76 Message M=l.34 M=1.61 M=1.10 M=1.35 g=27 g=22 p=11 g=8 59 Table 10. Means, standard deviations, and cell counts for Behaviors at Time 2 by message choice, message threat level, and sensation seeking. Choice of Message No Choice of Message Low Sensation High Sensation Low Sensation High Sensation Seekers Seekers Seekers Seekers High Threat a=4.46 )_(=3.9l a=4.92 _x=3.81 Message M=2.10 M=1.80 M= 1.88 M=1.74 a=14 p=12 g=17 p=30 Low Threat a=4.39 a=3.42 a= 5.39 a=3.57 Message M=1.88 M= 1.82 M= 1.58 M=2.23 2:27 p = 22 p = 11 g = 8 Table 11. Means, standard deviations, and cell counts for “Flossing more oflen” by message choice, message threat level, and sensation seeking. Choice of Message No Choice of Message Low Sensation High Sensation Low Sensation High Sensation Seekers Seekers Seekers Seekers High Threat x=3.32 a=301 x_=5.01 a=3.97 Message sd=2.11 M=1.53 M=1.83 M=1.88 p=14 a=12 p=17 p=30 Low Threat x=3.73 a: 3.32 a=4.43 _x=3.09 Message M =1.88 M = 2.02 M = 1.79 M =1.79 a: 27 p = 22 p = 11 a = 9 Table 12. Means, standard deviations, and cell counts for “Flossing more caretullz” by message choice, message threat level, and sensation seeking. Choice of Message No Choice of Message Low Sensation High Sensation Low Sensation High Sensation Seekers Seekers Seekers Seekers High Threat x=3.47 a=3.57 _x=4.73 x=3.81 Message s_ = 2.17 M = 1.61 M = 1.90 M =1.83 p=l4 p= 2 _=17 g=30 Low Threat a=3.57 5= .02 a=4.10 a=3.10 Message M= 1.77 M= 1.75 M= 1.41 M= 1.45 pfi= 27 a: 22 p = 11 p = 9 60 Table 13. Means, standard deviations, and cell counts for “Flossing more regularly” by message choice, message threat level, and sensation seeking. Choice of Message No Choice of Message Low Sensation High Sensation Low Sensation High Sensation Seekers Seekers Seekers Seekers High Threat x=3.65 a=2.67 )_r=4.93 5:381 Message sd=2.24 M= 1.51 M=1.80 M= 1.73 p=14 g=12 a=17 p=30 Low Threat a = 3.69 a = 2.97 a = 4.36 a = 2.76 Message M =1.77 M = 1.83 sd =1.70 M =1.58 p=26 g=22 n=11 g=9 CHAPTER 5 SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS 0_ve_rvi_evz This study investigated the influence of sensation seeking and message choice on responses to fear appeals. It was expected that (a) individuals who read a high threat message; (b) who are low sensation seekers; and (c) who received a preferred message would be more likely to have positive attitudes, to intend to engage in, and to perform a recommended response advocated in a fear appeal. Overall, the pattern of findings reveal that attitudes and intentions were seldom affected by sensation seeking or message choice; however, behaviors were affected. The lack of attitude change in this study may be due to the overall highly positive attitudes held initially by the participants. Initial attitudes towards flossing as a preventive measure were extremely positive and there was little variance in attitudes. Thus, the creation of more positive attitudes would need to be measured on a more sensitive scale than was utilized. In addition, these highly positive attitudes may be attributable to a prevailing understanding of the benefits associated with proper dental hygiene. The benefits of a regimen of proper brushing and flossing may be considered a cultural truism; that is, knowledge of these benefits is accepted and unquestioned by the vast majority of people within society (McGuire, 1961, 1962; McGuire & Papageorgis, 1961). Under this circumstance, strengthening already strong attitudes may be both diflicult to achieve and unnecessary in order to produce behavior change. 61 62 The remainder of this chapter discusses the main findings for each of the independent variables: message threat level, sensation seeking, and message choice. Next, implications of these findings in addressing the EPPM’s shortcomings are discussed. Finally, implications for health communicators based on the findings of this investigation are presented. Message Threat Level Consistent with previous fear appeal research, the high threat fear appeal message was more effective in producing the advocated behavior than the low threat message. The message in this investigation was a high threat, high efficacy message which would motivate people to control the danger posed by the health threat. This finding is in contrast to the findings of an investigation of dental hygiene compliance by Janis and Feshbach (1953). They found that the low threat fear appeal was more effective in producing compliance than was the high threat message. One explanation for these seemingly inconsistent results rest with the efficacy level conveyed by the message. Whereas the messages used in this investigation contained high efficacy components, the efficacy level conveyed by the messages in the Janis and Feshbach investigation is unknown. As Witte and Allen (1996) point out, high threat fear appeals are most efl‘ective when they are coupled with high efficacy components. Although attitudes were unaffected (most likely due to a ceiling effect), individuals who read the high threat message were more likely to engage in the recommended response (i.e., floss their teeth) than were individuals who read the low threat message. 63 Again, this lack of attitude change may be due to the strong positive attitudes people held initially and may be attributed to the reasons discussed in the previous section. Although the high threat message did not produce attitude change, it did lead individuals to follow proper dental hygiene suggestions more closely. The high threat message may have been a stronger reminder than the low threat message and served as an impetus to establish a efficacious habit or as an impetus to re-establish a lapsed habit. mama—king Sensation Seeking and Choice. Contrary to expectations, high sensation seekers were no more likely to choose to view the high threat message than were low sensation seekers. Thus, we may expect the majority of the population, regardless of sensation seeking level, to prefer messages which are unlikely to arouse high levels of fear. However, although individuals may not prefer high threat messages, these messages are the ones which are more effective in promoting compliance with behavioral recommendations. Sensation seeking, as a general trait, was unrelated to choice of either a high threat or a low threat message. However, analyses suggest that two of the four factors which comprise sensation seeking -- boredom susceptibility and disinhibition - may be related to message threat level preference. First, those individuals in the upper tertile of boredom susceptibility were more likely to choose the low threat message. This may be due to the options provided. The low threat message was described as a “message designed to arouse curiosity”. This finding may indicate a tendency for those high in boredom susceptibility to desire more mental stimulation associated with cognitive, rather than 64 emotional, arousal. When posed with a choice between explicit and scary material and material designed to arouse curiosity, those high in boredom susceptibility may be attracted to a message which may pique interest and stimulate thought rather than one which invokes fear. Similarly, those who may be more likely to choose a high threat message described as “explicit and scary” may be those who are high in disinhibition. This group, aside from describing individuals who seek unconventional social activities, and social stimulation associated with drugs, alcohol, and sex, may also include those who are intrigued by gruesome, graphic media depictions. They may also be more likely to desire to view graphic horror films, pornographic material, or extreme violence. Although the relationship between disinhibition and choice was not statistically significant, this may be due to the weak reliability of the items measuring disinhibition. Sensation Seeking. Attitudes. Intentions and Behaviors. Previous research has shown that low sensation seekers have more positive attitudes and intentions and would be more likely to engage in the recommended response than high sensation seekers (Clayton, Cattarello, & Walden, 1991; Harrington & Donohew, 1996; Witte & Morrison, 1995). Although few significant differences emerged for attitudes and intentions across the four sensation seeking factors, those that did were consistent with previous research (i.e., individuals low in that sensation seeking factor had more positive attitudes and intentions). It is important to note that, although attitudes were not related to sensation seeking (except for boredom susceptibility at the two-week follow-up), behaviors were. 65 Sensation seeking may affect attitudes, intentions, and behaviors only when the activity has high sensation value (i.e., able to elicit strong sensory, affective reactions). The relationship between sensation seeking and engaging in an activity may depend upon the type and general riskiness of the activity involved such that only those risk activities with high perceived risk are likely to result in differential attitudes, intentions, and behaviors across sensation seeking levels. Activities such as bungee jumping, sky diving, drugs, travel to exotic places, unprotected sex, or other high risk activities may provide the biologically based stimulation desired by the high sensation seekers. Activities such as flossing one’s teeth, taking vitamins, and wearing sunscreen daily may be beneficial to one’s health but not provide enough stimulation such that high sensation seekers might be more likely to engage in the risky behavior. Moreover, different activities may not satisfy different biologically driven needs as reflected in the four factors which comprise sensation seeking. For example, a roller coaster ride may satisfy a need to engage in fiightening experiences and be related to an individuals’ inclination toward thrill and adventure seeking; however, it may not satisfy an inclination toward experience seeking and a desire to engage in novel experiences. Message Choice Counter to what was expected, individuals who received the message they preferred did not have more positive attitudes, intentions, and behaviors than those who did not receive the message they preferred. To the contrary, those who received the message they rejected had more positive behaviors. (As mentioned previously, attitudes and intentions were unaffected.) This may be due to the effectiveness of the high threat 66 message producing more positive behaviors in general as compared to the low threat message. In this study, participants received one of two messages: a high threat or low threat message. Those who chose to view the low threat message, but did not receive it, were exposed to the high threat message. As discussed above, the high threat message produced more positive behaviors. Moreover, this group (which requested the low threat but received the high threat message) reported flossing more often and more regularly than the group which chose and received the high threat message. Thus, it appears that individuals who desire low threat messages but are exposed to high threat messages are still more likely to change their behaviors, even though their “choice” has, in effect, been taken away. It appears that if individuals have a choice initially, even if that preference cannot be satisfied, will lead to positive behaviors. Interestingly, individuals who chose the high threat message reported greater susceptibility to the health threat than did those who chose the low threat message. It may be that those who believe themselves to be at greater risk to harm fiom a health threat search for information which will be more likely to affirm this belief. To the extent that high threat fear appeals, messages which are “explicit and scary,” are likely to confirm this belief, individuals who believe themselves to be at greater risk will seek out health messages, especially those which have the potential to confirm their beliefs about their susceptibility to the threat. Because they already have high perceived susceptibility, we might expect that this group to have more positive attitudes and intentions to engage in health protective behaviors when exposed to high threat messages containing strong efficacy messages. 67 MaaMge Threat Level. Sensation Seemgand Message Choice The expected additive effects for choice, threat level, and sensation seeking were not found; rather, more positive attitudes, intentions, and behaviors were found for those individuals who chose the low threat but received the high threat message. Again, this may be attributed to the greater effectiveness of a high threat, high efficacy fear appeal. The influence of sensation seeking may have been absent due to the relatively low degree of risk associated with not engaging in the recommended response to prevent periodontal disease. However, if sensation seeking is truly a personality trait, then its effects should be observed regardless of the health topic chosen. The results within this study suggest that the utility of sensation seeking as an individual difference variable is limited by the behavior under investigation. In addition, given the results of this investigation, it is noted that the four factors which constitute sensation seeking (e. g., Thrill and Adventure Seeking; Experience Seeking; Disinhibition; and Boredom Susceptibility) represent different aspects of personality and do not necessarily reflect the same underlying latent personality trait. The analyses conducted suggest that, although the four factors are related, each is conceptually distinct and may be indicative of difi‘erent underlying biologically based needs. In other words, each factor reflects desires for different types of stimulation and each factor may have a different relationship with the outcome under investigation. This suggests that, in the firture, analyses by each factor separately may be more illuminating and provide more insight about the relationship among the theoretical constructs than would analysis by the sensation seeking factor as a whole. 68 I_rr_ipl_thions for the Extended Pmllel Process Model The EPPM served as the theoretical basis for this investigation. The EPPM suggests that perceptions of threat determine if an individual will be motivated to take health protective action while perceptions of efficacy determine the nature of that action. According to the EPPM, high threat, high efficacy messages will be the most efi‘ective in gaining compliance. Two shortcomings of the EPPM were identified: (a) the lack of attention given to individual difference variables and their potential impact on message acceptance; and (b) individuals’ propensity to selectively expose themselves to information. Sensation seeking was examined because it has been found to influence desire for high threat messages as well as to reduce the likelihood of engaging in health protective action. Results of this study suggest that, with efficacy held constant at a high level, high threat messages are more effective in gaining behavioral compliance than are low threat messages. These results are consistent with previous fear appeal studies. This study also demonstrated that most people prefer to expose themselves to low threat messages. Thus, although high threat messages will be more effective in promoting behavioral change, there exists a need to address the role of selective exposure in the EPPM. Sensation seeking was identified as an individual difference variable which might explain selective exposure. Taken as a whole, sensation seeking does not appear to drive people’s choice of messages. This may be due to the weak reliability of the scale items as well as the evidence that the four factors which constitute sensation seeking (e. g., Thrill and Adventure Seeking; Experience Seeking; Disinhibition; and Boredom Susceptibility) 69 represent different aspects of personality and do not necessarily represent the same underlying personality trait. In addition, sensation seeking does not appear to be related to engaging in health protective action for activities lacking high sensation value. Taken together, these results suggest that it is possible certain sensation seeking factors to be better predictors of selective exposure to high threat messages yet be unrelated to the propensity to engage in risky activities. Again, the propensity to engage in health protective action may rest on the type of activity being advocated. Results also suggest that the people most likely to selectively expose themselves to high threat messages are those who perceive themselves susceptible to the health risk. For this group, it is expected that high threat, high efficacy messages will be effective. People who feel susceptible and seek out information which affirrns this belief are likely to encounter information about how they can change their behavior to protect themselves. Active information seeking behavior may also indicate readiness and motivation to adopt recommended courses of action. Yet, these results also raise the possibility that this group will be more likely to engage in danger control responses (i.e., attitude, intention, and behavior change) regardless of the level of efficacy contained within the message. This investigation suggests that selective exposure to a high threat message may play a pivotal role in determining the likelihood of acceptance of the message’s recommendations. Although sensation seeking does not appear to provide a strong explanation of selective exposure, perceived susceptibility does. The factors influencing susceptibility perceptions may hold the key to understanding when and why individuals are 7O likely to expose themselves to high threat messages and to engage in danger control processes. linpliflttions for Health Communicators Health communicators who want to spur health protective action may want to utilize high threat fear appeals with high efficacy components. It is these messages which have consistently been found to be most effective (Rogers, 1983; Witte & Allen, 1996). This investigation provides two reasons why high threat fear appeals with high efficacy messages would be most effective. First, this study suggests that most people prefer to expose themselves to low threat messages. However, these messages are likely to be less effective in promoting attitude, intention, and behavior change in than would a high threat message. Yet, when individuals who desire a low threat message receive a high threat message, they have more positive attitudes, intentions and behaviors. Thus, use of a high threat fear appeal should prove more effective for these individuals. Second, this study also suggests that those individuals who desired to expose themselves to a high threat message perceived themselves to be more susceptible to the health threat. Believing that they are at risk for a health threat and that they are able to easily and effectively avert the threat may motivate these individuals to engage in health protective action. A caveat to this approach revolves around the augmenting of perceived susceptibility for individuals who may not necessarily be at greater risk to a health threat. The ramifications of increasing people’s perceived susceptibility to a threat for which they are not truly at risk must be taken into account. First, this may cause an individual to engage in a health protective action which is unnecessary. Second, it raises questions 71 about the ethics of using a high threat message regardless of the circumstances. Third, if this approach is taken, the message must include messages which increase perceived response efficacy and perceived self-efficacy to create conditions under which people will be most likely to perform actions which would not prove detrimental to their health. Although use of a high threat fear appeal is effective, it leaves the potential to increase the desire of high sensation seekers to engage in the risky activity. Indeed, previous research has shown that high sensation seekers are more likely to engage in the discouraged activity (e. g., Clayton, Cattarello, & Walden, 1991; Harrington & Donohew, 1996; Witte & Morrison, 1995). As Witte and Morrison (1995) suggest, when using high threat fear appeal messages the campaign needs to include a substitute behavior which will provide the stimulation desired by high sensation seekers but will not lead to the negative consequences. If a substitute behavior is not provided, the high sensation seeker is unlikely to perform the recommended response and the high threat fear appeal may provide an impetus to engage in the risky behavior. Finally, health communicators must give consideration to the nature of the health threat and the recommended responses. When advocating the creation, rather than the cessation of habits, additional avenues to encourage maintenance must be provided. Under most circumstances, the creation of a habit (like flossing one’s teeth) may involve more effort than would the cessation of an activity because it requires the establishment of patterns which may be viewed as not entirely within the realm of one’s control. Although one may have a positive attitude, behavioral performance may not occur because one does not believe that the performance of that behavior will be easy (Ajzen & Madden, 1986; 72 McCaul, Sandgren, O’Neill, & Hinsz, 1993). Not only would one message be insuflicient to bring about attitude, intention, and behavior change, but strategies to encourage behavior change as well as maintenance of the habit and long term performance would be necessary for continued performance of the recommended response (McCaul, Glasgow & O’Neill, 1992). Simply advising people what to do, without providing the knowledge of how to perform the behavior as well as the means to support the creation of the habit may leave motivated individuals otherwise unable to perform the desired actions. Health communication campaigns which are limited solely to media messages may be insufficient to spur health protective action. Utilization of peers and interpersonal communication may prove fruitful in developing the advocated behaviors. Summm Fear appeals have been the subject of much investigation over the past forty years. Results of these studies indicate that fear appeals are potent messages, especially when they utilize both high threat and high efficacy components. This investigation provides fiirther evidence for the effectiveness of high threat/high efiicacy messages regardless of an individual difference variable (i.e., sensation seeking) and regardless of a person’s desire to view a high threat message. In sum, campaigners must make audience members feel at risk to a serious health threat and make them feel that they can easily and effectively avert it. LIST OF REFERENCES REFERENCES Ajzen, I., & F ishbein, M. (1977). Attitude-behavior relations: A theoretical analysis and review of empirical research. Psychological Bulletin. 84. 888-918. Ajzen, I., & Madden, T. J. (1986). Prediction of goal-directed behavior: Attitudes, intentions, and perceived behavioral control. J 01ml of Experimental Social Psychology, 22, 453-474. Amett, J. (1994). Sensation seeking: A new conceptualization and a new scale. Personality and Individe Differences 16(2). 289-296. Atkin, C. K. (1994). Designing persuasive health messages. In L. Sechrest, T. E. Backer, E. M. Rodgers, T. F. Campbell, & M. L. Grady (Eds), Effecaive diaaemination of elinical and health information, pp. 99-106. Rockville, MD: U. S. Dept. of Health and Human Services (AHCPR Pub. No. 95-0015). Atkin, C. K. (1985). Informational utility and selective exposure to entertainment media. In D. Zillman & J. Bryant (Eds) Selective exposure to communication , pp. 63-91. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Atkin, C. K., & Freimuth, V. (1989). Formative evaluation research in campaign design. In R. E. Rice & C. K. Atkin (Eds) Publie communication campaigps (2nd ed.), pp. 131-150. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Beck, K. H., & Lund, A. K. (1981). The effects of health threat seriousness and personal efficacy upon intentions and behavior. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 11 (a), 401-415. 73 74 Berkowitz, J. M., Lapinski, M. K., Liu, W., Cameron, K. A., McKeon, J. K., & Witte, K. (1996). MaMpgsense of health risks: The acquisition of knowledge about radon. Unpublished manuscript. Boster, F. J ., & Mongeau, P. (1984). Fear-arousing persuasive messages. In R Bostrom (Ed), Communication yearbook. vol. 8.. (pp. 330-375). Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Bhaskar, S. N. (1986). Synopsis of Oral Pathology (7th ed). Toronto: C. V. Mosby. Burnett, J. J. (1981). Intemal-extemal locus of control as a moderator of fear appeals. Journal of Applied Psychology, 66, 390-393. Cacioppo, J. T., & Petty, R. E. (1982). The need for cognition. Journal of Personam a_nd Social Psychology,4_2, 116-131. Cacioppo, J. T., Petty, R. E., Kao, C. F., & Rodriguez, R., (1986). Central and peripheral routes to persuasion: An individual difference perspective. Journal of Personally and Social Pg;chology,_5l, 1032-1043. Camner, L. G., Sandell, R., & Sarhed, G. (1994). The role of patient involvement in oral hygiene compliance. Britiah Journal of Clinical Psychology,_3;, 379-390. Carranza, F. A. (1990). Glickman’s Clinical Periodontology (7th ed). Philadelphia: W. B. Saunders. Casey, M. K. (1995). Fatalism and the modification of the extended pa_rallel process _m_d<_lal. Paper presented at the meeting of the Speech Communication Association, San Antonio, TX. 75 Chanowitz, B., & Langer, E. J. (1993). Premature cognitive commitment. Journal of Personalig and Social Psychology, 41 (6), 1051-1063. Clayton, R. R., Cattarello, A., & Walden, K. P. (1991). Sensation seeking as a potential mediating variable for school-based prevention intervention: A two-year follow- up of DARE. Elaalth Communigrtion. 3 (4). 229-239. Clayton, R. R., Leukefeld, C. G., Donohew, L., Bardo, M., & Harrington, N. G. (1995). Risk and protective factors: A brief review. Drugs & Society. 8 (3-4). 7-14. Covello, V. T., von Winterfeldt, D., & Slovic, P. (1987). Risk communication: A review of the literature. Risk Abstracts, 171-177. Cross, W. G. (1977). Gingivitis (2nd ed). Bristol: John Wright & Sons. Dembroski, T. M., Lasater, T. M., & Ramirez, A. (1978). Communicator similarity, fear arousing communications, and compliance with health care recommendations. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 8 (3 ), 254-269. Dillard, J. P. (1994). Rethinking the study of fear appeals: An emotional perspective. Communication Theory. 4. 295-323. Donohew, L., Firm, 8., & Christ, W. G. (1988). “The nature of news” revisited: The roles of affect, schemas, and cognition. In L. Donohew, H. E. Sypher, & E. T. Higgins (Eds), Communication. social cognition. and affect, pp. 195-218. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Donohew, L., Lorch, E. P., & Palmgreen, P. (1991). Sensation seeking and targeting of televised anti-drug PSAs. In L. Donohew, H. E. Sypher, & W. Bukowski (Eds), 76 Persu_a_sive communication and drugabuse prevenm (pp. 209-226). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Donohew, L., Palmgreen, P., & Duncan, J. (1980). An activation model of information exposure. Communication Monographs, 47, 295-303. Donohew, L., & Tipton, L. (1973). A conceptual model of information seeking, avoiding, and processing. In P. Clarke (Ed) New models for mass communication r_eae_aac_l_r, pp. 243-268. Beverly Hills: Sage. Evans, R. I., Rozelle, R. M., Noblitt, R. & Williams, D. L. (1975). Explicit and implicit communications over time to initiate and maintain behavior change: New perspective utilizing a real-life dental hygiene situation. Journal of Applied Social Paychology, 5 (2), 150-156. Everett, M. W., & Palmgreen, P. (1995). Influences of sensation seeking, message sensation value, and program context on effectiveness of anticocaine public service announcements. Health CommaMication. 7 (3). 225-248. Finnegan, J. R., Jr.. (1994). Communicating health information to the public: The problem of exposure. In L. Sechrest, T. E. Backer, E. M. Rodgers, T. F. Campbell, & M. L. Grady (Eds), Effective dissemination of clinical_ and healah information. Rockville, MD: U. S. Dept. of Health and Human Services (AHCPR Pub. No. 95-0015). Fiske, S. T., & Taylor, S. E. (1991). Social Coalition. New York: McGraw-Hill, Inc. 77 Freimuth, V. 8., Hammond, S. L., Edgar, T., & Monahan, J. L. (1990). Reaching those at risk: A content-analytic study of AIDS PSAs. Communication Research. 17 (6). 775-791. Hale, J. L., & Dillard, J. P. (1995). Fear appeals in health promotion campaigns: Too much, too little, or just right? In E. Maibach & R. L. Parrott (Eds) Desigmng Health Messages: Approaches from Communication Theory and Public Heaala mam (pp. 65-80). Thousands Oaks, CA: Sage. Harrington, N. G., & Donohew, L. (1996). JUMP START: A targeted substance abuse prevention program. Manuscript submitted for publication. Horowitz, I. A. (1972). Attitude change as a function of perceived arousal. Journal of Social Psycholdgy, 87, 117-126. Horowitz, I. A. (1969). Effects of volunteering, fear arousal, and number of communications on attitude change. Journal of Persoaality Md Social Psychalegy, Mu), 34-3 7. Horowitz, I. A., & Gumenik, W. E. (1970). Effects of the volunteer subject, choice, and fear arousal on attitude change. Jorflal of Experimental Social Psycholo 6, 293-303. Horvath, P., & Zuckerrnan, M. (1993). Sensation seeking, risk appraisal, and risky behavior. Personalig and Individual Differencea,1_4(_l_), 41-52. Hunter, J. E., & Gerbing, D. W. (1982). Unidimensional measurement, second order factor analysis, and causal models. Research in Organizational Behavior. 4. 267- 320. 78 Janis, I. L., & Feshbach, S. (1953). Effects of fear arousing communications. Journal of Abnormal & Social Psychology, 48 (1), 78-92. Janis, I. L., & F eshbach, S. (1954). Personality differences associated with responsiveness to fear-arousing communications. Journal of Personality, 23 (1), 154-166. Johnson, J. D., & Meischke, H. (1993). A comprehensive model of cancer-related information seeking applied to magazines. Human Comm_unication Research. 19 (3), pp. 343-367. Kalichman, S. C., Heckman, T., & Kelly, J. A. (1996). Sensation seeking as an explanation for the association between substance use and HIV-related risky sexual behavior. Archives of Sexaal Behavior. 25 (2). 141-141-154. Kalichman, S. C., & Rompa, D. (1995). Sexual sensation seeking and sexual compulsivity scales: Reliability, validity, and predicting HIV risk behavior. Journal of Personalng Assessment, 65 (3 ), 586-601. Keinan, G., Meir, E., & Gome-Nemirovsky, T. (1984). Measurement of risk takers’ personality. Psychological Reports. 55. 16 Lamey, P., & Lewis, M. A. O. (1988). Oral medicine. New York: Gower Medial Publishing. Leventhal, H. (1970). Findings and theory in the study of fear communications. In L. Berkowitz (Ed), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 5, pp. 119- 186). New York: Academic Press. 79 Lorch, E. P., Palmgreen, P., Donohew, L., Helm, D., Baer, S. A., Dsilva, M. U. (1994). Program context, sensation seeking, and attention to televised anti-drug public service announcements. Humaflommpnication Research. 20 (3). 390-412. McCallum, D. 3., Hammond, S. L., & Covello, V. T. (1991). Communicating about environmental risks: How the public uses and perceives information sources. Health Education Quarterly, 18 (3), 349-361. McCaul, K. D., O’Neill, H. K., & Glasgow, R. E. (1988). Predicting the performance of dental hygiene behaviors: An examination of the F ishbein and Ajzen model and self-efficacy expectations. Journal of Applied Secial Psycholggy, 18 (2), 114-128. McCaul, K. D., Sandgren, A. K., O’Neill, H. K., & Hinsz, V. B. (1993). The value of the theory of planned behavior, perceived control, and self-efficacy expectations for predicting health-protective behaviors. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 14 (a), 231-252. McGuire, W. J. (1961). Resistance to persuasion conferred by active and passive prior refutation of the same and alternative counterarguments. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychologyfi, 326-332. McGuire, W. J. (1962). Persistence of the resistance to persuasion induced by various types of prior defenses. Joumalaof Abnormal_an_d Social Psychologyfi, 241-248. McGuire, W. J. (1989). Theoretical foundations of campaigns. In R. E. Rice & C. K. Atkin (Eds) Public communication campaigns (2nd ed), pp. 43-66. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. 80 McGuire, W. J ., & Papageorgis, D. (1961). The relative efficacy of various types of prior beliefs-defense in producing immunity against persuasion. J ouMal of Abnormal and Social Psychology,_62, 327-337. Mongeau, P. (1994). Another look at fear arousing persuasive appeals. In M. Allen & R. Preiss (Eds), Prospects and precautions in the use of meta-analysis (pp. 75-100). Dubuque, IA: William C. Brown. Newcomb, M. D., & McGee, L. (1991). Influence of sensation seeking on general deviance and specific problem behaviors from adolescence to young adulthood. Journal of Perscfllity and Social Psychology, 61 (4), 614-628. Olson, P. (1993). Consumer Behavior. Boston: Irwin. O’Keefe, D. J. (1990). Persuasion: Theoryand research. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Palmgreen, P., Donohew, L., Lorch, E. P., Rogus, M., Helm, D., & Grant, N. (1991). Sensation seeking, message sensation value, and drug use as mediators of PSA effectiveness. filth Communicatioa 3 (4). 217-227. Palmgreen, P., Lorch, E. P., Donohew, L., Harrington, N. G., Dsilva, M., & Helm, D. (1995). Reaching at-risk populations in a mass media drug abuse prevention campaign: Sensation seeking as a targeting variable. Drugs & Socieg, 8 (3-4), 29-45. Rodriguez, J. I. (1995). Confounds in fear arousing persuasive messages: Do the paths less traveled make all the difference? Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Michigan State University, East Lansing, MI. 81 Rogers, R. (1983). Cognitive and physiological processes in fear appeals and attitude change: A revised theory of protection motivation. In J. Cacioppo & R. Petty (Eds), Social Psychophysiology (pp. 153-176). New York: Guilford. Rosenstock, I. M., Strecher, V. J ., & Becker, M. H. (1988). Social learning theory and the health belief model. flMlth Educaaion Quarterly, 15 (2), 175-183. Schlosberg, S. (1996). Brushing up. Health (March/April), 104-106. Sheer, V. C. (1995). Sensation seeking predispositions and susceptibility to a sexual partner’s appeals for condom use. Journal of Applied Comm_unication Research. M, 212-229. Sheer, V. C., & Cline, R. J. (1994). The development and validation of a model explaining sexual behavior among college students: Implications for AIDS communication campaigns. Humap Communiaation Researeh, 21 (2), 280-304. Sheer, V. C., & Cline, R. J. W. (1995). Individual differences in sensation seeking and sexual behavior: Implications for communication intervention for HIV/AIDS prevention among college students. Harlth Comm_unication. 7 (3). 205-223. Smith, S. (1995). Perceptual processing of nonverbal-relational messages. In D. E. Hewes (Ed) The Cagnitive Bases of Interpersora) Communication (pp. 87-112). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Snyder, M. (1982). When believing means doing: Creating links between attitudes and behavior. IN M. P. Zanna, E. T. Higgins, & C. P. Herman (Eds), Conaistency in Social Behavior: The Ontario Smposium, vol. 2 (pp. 105-130). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. 82 Sutton, S. R. (1982). F ear-arousing communication: A critical examination of theory and research. In J. R. Eiser (Ed), Social psychology and behavioral medicine (pp. 303-337). London: John Wiley. Watt, J. H., & van den Berg, S. A. (1995). Research Methods for Comm_unication Science. Boston: Allyn and Bacon. Weinstein, N. D. (1989). Perceptions of personal susceptibility to harm. In. V. M. Mays, G. W. Albee, & S. F. Schneider (Eds) Primapy Prevention of AIDS: PMchological Approaches. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Wilson, T. 6., Jr. (1989). Disruption or reduction of bacterial populations in the oral cavity. In T. G. Wilson, Jr. (ed) Dental mainteMnce for patients with periodontal Mes; Chicago: Quintessence. Wilson, T. G., Jr., & Komman, K. S. (1996). Fundamentals of Periodonm. Chicago: Quintessence. Witte, K. (1994). Fear and danger control: A test of the Extended Parallel Process Model (EPPM). Communication Monoggaphs, 61 (2), 113-134. Witte, K. (1992a). Message and conceptual confounds in fear appeals: The role of threat, fear, and efficacy. The Southern Commanication Journal. 58. 147-155. Witte, K. (1992b). Putting the fear back into fear appeals: The Extended Parallel Process Model. Communication Monographs, 59, 330-349. Witte, K., & Allen, M. (1996). When do scare tactics worlMA meta-analysis of fear appeals. Manuscript submitted for publication. 83 Witte, K., Berkowitz, J. M., Cameron, K. A., & McKeon, J. K. (1996). Preventing the apread of genital warts: Using fear appeals to promote self-protective behaviors. Manuscript submitted for publication. Witte, K., & Morrison, K. (1995). Using scare tactics to promote safer sex among juvenile detention and high school youth. Jouml of Applied Comflmication Research. 23. 1-15. Witte, K., Peterson, T. R., Vallabhan, S., Stephenson, M. T., Plugge, C. D., Givens, V. K., Todd, J. D., Becktold, M. G., Hyde, M. K., & Jarrett, R. (1993). Preventing tractor-related injuries and deaths in rural populations: Using a persuasive health message fiamework in formative evaluation research. International Quarterly of Qdmmunig Health Education, 13 (3), 219-251. Zuckerrnan, M. (1994). Bahaviora expressions and biosociabases of sensation seelg'ag. New York: Cambridge University Press. Zuckerrnan, M. (1988). Behavior and biology: Research on sensation seeking and reactions to the media. In L. Donohew, H. E. Sypher, & E. T. Higgins (Eds), Communication. Social Cognition. and Affect. pp. 173-194. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Zuckerrnan, M. (1979). Beyond the optinaal level of aaousal. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Zuckerrnan, M. (1971). Dimensions of sensation seeking. Journal of ConsultingMd Clinieal Psychology, 36 (1), 45-52. 84 Zuckerrnan, M., Ball, 8., & Black, J. (1990). Influences of sensation seeking, gender, risk appraisal, and situational motivation on smoking. Addictive Behaviors. 15. 209- 220. Zuckerrnan, M., Eysenck, S., & Eysenck, H. J. (1978). Sensation seeking in England and America: Cross-cultural, age, and sex comparisons. Jouml of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 46 (1), 139-149. Zuckerrnan, M., Kolin, E. A., Price, L., & Zoob, I. (1964). Development of a sensation- seeking scale. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 28 (6), 477-481. Zuckerrnan, M., & Link, K. (1968). Construct validity for the sensation-seeking scale. Jomal of Consulting Psychology, 32 (4), 420-426. MICHIGAN sran UNIV. LIBRARIES ll"Willillllllllllllllll”Illlllllllllllllllll 31293017663174