THESIS llllllllllllllllllllllllllllllllllllllllllll 193 01778 5670 LIBRARY Michigan State University This is to certify that the dissertation entitled GROUP-BASED RIVALRY: A SOCIAL CONFLICT THEORY ON THE INFLUENCE OF GROUP MEMBERSHIP ON COMPETITIVE BEHAVIOR presented by Leslie McClintock Steel has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for Ph.D. degreein H.E.D. 4 / -72? ,//l ‘flf’ I. . l'r 52’; w/fx [(7): 2'29 J4. f Major professor ¢./ x.‘_ v \ ’ .-" /, .r /( ‘(7/7 Date/‘( (.(‘Z'fi'xn /{:j /.é/ / /// MS U is an Affirmative Action/[q ual Opportunity Institution 0-12771 PLACE iN RETURN Box to remove this checkout from your record. TO AVOID FINES return on or before date due. DATE DUE DATE DUE DATE DUE 1198 «mm-mu GROUP-BASED RIVALRY: A SOCIAL CONFLICT THEORY ON THE INFLUENCE OF GROUP MEMBERSHIP ON COMPETITIVE BEHAVIOR By Leslie McClintock Stoel A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Human Environment: Design and Management Merchandising Management Specialization 1997 ABSTRACT GROUP-BASED RIVALRY: A SOCIAL CONFLICT THEORY ON THE INFLUENCE OF GROUP MEMBERSHIP ON COMPETITIVE BEHAVIOR By Leslie McClintock Stoel A new form of competition, group versus group, is emerging in many industries as firms form groups to increase their competitive edge in the marketplace. This paper develops a theory to explain the effect group membership has on inter-group competitive behavior and its outcomes. Social conflict theory is used as the basis from which to derive the proposed theory. Rivalry research provides support for propositions concerning behaviors of individuals in competitive situations. Group research provides the foundation for proposing the effects of group membership on behavior. The merging of social conflict theory, rivalry research and group research provides a new method of examining the influence of group membership on competitive behavior. The research found that, for retail cooperative members, group member identification with the group of affiliation resulted in an increased awareness of goal conflicts with rivals, and, that feelings of group identification inspired action of group members, manifested in an increased use of rivalrous behaviors. Contrary to expectations, increased awareness of goal conflict did not influence the use of rivalrous behaviors, and higher levels of rivalrous behaviors did not relate to financial performance. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS A large part of my success in completing this degree is due to several important groups of people. The first group is my Committee. I’d like to thank Dr. Brenda Stemquist, the Chair of my Committee, for her endless support of my teaching and research. Thanks to Dr. Linda Good and Dr. Patricia Huddleston for being ten'ific mentors throughout my program. Thanks to Dr. Glenn Omura, in Marketing and Logistics, for his creativity and his interest and generosity in helping turn creative ideas into real projects. Although not on my committee, I must thank Dr. Roger Calantone, in Marketing and Logistics, for his assistance with structural equation modeling problems. The second group to acknowledge is my family. Thanks to my husband Dale, for enduring this adventure with me, to my parents Russ and Carol McClintock, for their uplifting support, and to Dale’s parents, Bob and Mary Stoel, for their support also. And, the last group to mention is the rest of the Doctoral Students in the new Merchandising Management program: Jae Eun Chung, Madeline Flaster, Barb Frazier, Linda Niehm, Linda Plank, Vanessa Wicklifl‘e. Thanks to all of them for the camaraderie, the “phiIOSOphical” discussions, and the support as we progressed through the program. iii TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF FIGURES ................................................... vi LIST OF TABLES ................................................... vii Chapter 1 Introduction ......................................................... 1 Significance of Study ............................................. 1 Problem Definition ............................................... 3 Chapter 2 Literature Review ..................................................... 5 Rivalry ........................................................ 5 Group Research ................................................. 6 Theoretical Framework ........................................... 7 Social Conflict Theory ...................................... 7 Theory Development ............................................. 8 Causes/Antecedents ........................................ 9 Behavior ................................................ 11 Outcomes ............................................... 14 Chapter 3 Methodology ........................................................ 18 Measures ..................................................... 18 Group Identification ....................................... 18 Goal Conflict ............................................ 18 Behavior ................................................ l9 Outcome ............................................... 20 Sample ....................................................... 21 Data Collection ................................................ 22 Data Analysis .................................................. 25 Chapter 4 Results ............................................................ 26 Measurement Model ............................................ 26 Structural Model ............................................... 29 Hypotheses ................................................... 29 iv Chapter 5 Conclusion ......................................................... 33 Implications ................................................... 34 Limitations and Future Research ................................... 35 References .......................................................... 38 Appendix A. Patterns and Dimensions of Rivalry ............................ 58 Appendix B. Questionnaire ............................................. 64 Appendix C. Formulas for Composite Reliability and Variance Extracted .......... 68 LIST OF FIGURES Figure 1. General Conflict Model ........................................ 44 Figure 2. Theoretical Model ............................................ 45 Figure 3. Initial Model ................................................ 46 Figure 4. Revised Model ............................................... 47 LIST OF TABLES Table 1. Item Content: Group Identification ................................ 48 Table 2. Item Content: Goal Conflict ...................................... 49 Table 3. Item Content: Behavior ......................................... 50 Table 4. Item Content: Outcome ......................................... 51 Table 5. Sample Characteristics ......................................... 52 Table 6. Representativeness of Sample .................................... 53 Table 7. Non-response Bias ............................................ 54 Table 8. Measurement Model Results ..................................... 55 Table 9. Structural Model .............................................. 57 vii Chapter 1 Introduction Significance of Study In 1995, the top one hundred U.S. retailers averaged sales increases of 10.4% with profit decreases of 24.4% over 1994 (Schulz 1996a). Some industry observers contend that the current environment is the most difficult since the early 19705 (Johnson and Mendelson 1996). Because of an “intensive wave of bankruptcies, mergers and store closings” in 1995 (Schulz 1996a, p. 56), fewer, larger competitors remain to capture consumer dollars. An examination of 1982, 1987 and 1992 retail concentration ratios does confirm that smaller players are losing market Share to larger players. The four largest retailers controlled 5.5% of retail sales in 1982, 5.2% in 1987, and increased their share to 6.8% in 1992. The fifiy largest retail firms captured 19.8% of retail sales in 1982, 20.3% in 1987, and 22.4% in 1992 (U .S. Department of Commerce 1995, 1990, 1985). Yet even large size does not guarantee success in this trying environment. In the fourth quarter of 1995, Wal-mart, the nation’s largest retailer, recorded its first earnings decline in 99 quarters (“State of the Industry,” 1996). What effect will such intense competition have on retailers and the retail industry? What can retailers do to survive such intense competition? 2 For many years, firms have used partnering strategies to compete in threatening environments. These collaborative partnering efforts, often called strategic alliances, take many different forms, ranging from “the simplest transactional relationship to licensing and outsourcing to anything short of a merger” (Maynard 1996, p. 19). To help classify these partnering strategies, Maynard proposed a spectrum of collaborative business relationships based on degree of collaboration, level of formality and amount of expected added value. For example, low levels of collaboration and formality characterize external, tactical transactions, such as arm’s length agreements with vendors and equity investments, and result in a small amount of added value to the partners. Extended partner networks, such as buying cooperatives, franchise agreements and joint ventures, require higher levels of formality and collaboration to execute than tactical transactions. We call these extended relationships strategic alliances because the partnering firms rely on the capture of interfirrn synergies to provide added value. Mergers and acquisitions are examples of partnerships using internal growth to add value. These strategies allow partners to realize synergies and share risks and benefits by internalizing them (Gomez-Casseres 1994, Maynard 1996). Estimates show that between 1986 and 1996, the number of alliance partnerships, as defined in the spectrum proposed by Maynard, increased 25% annually (Maynard 1996). The result of the popularity of alliance partnering for competing in dificult environments is that, in many industries, multiple groups are beginning to appear. In other words, a new form of competition is emerging: “group versus group.” (Gomez-Casseres 1994, p. 62). Industry observers believe that increased formation of alliance groups will intensify 3 industry competition (Gomez-Casseres 1994). The challenge of this new competitive situation is to understand how group membership influences the competitive behaviors of group members. Developing an understanding of group-based competition is a necessary skill for survival in today’s marketplace. The purpose of this paper is to develop a theory of inter-group competitive behavior that will enable groups in an industry to better understand their competitive environment. Problem Definition In the fierce economy of the 19905, organizations are turning to group-based methods of competing to fully capitalize on their resources. These organizations are exploring largely unknown territory because theories that explain and predict dynamic, group-based competition do not exist. In addition, little empirical evidence exists to explain the effect that group-based competition has on firms, groups or the industry. This paper seeks to solve this problem by developing a theory that will propose the effects of group membership on competitive behaviors and the outcomes of those behaviors. Recent empirical studies in the strategy literature (Chen 1996a; Chen 1996b; Chen and Hambrick 1995; Smith, et a1. 1997) have added to our understanding of competitive behaviors of firms by analyzing the conduct of individual pairs of specific firms in a single industry. Strategists term this dyadic behavior rivalry. We distinguish rivalry, the competitive interactions of individual pairs of Specific firms, fiom competition, a general term describing actions of nonspecific firms in a market. By distinguishing between rivalry and competition, strategy researchers have provided a more effective method of explaining 4 and predicting causes and characteristics of competitive behavior than economic theories and competitor analysis. This paper will apply the construct of rivalry to group-based behavior to develop an effective method of analyzing group-based competition. I derive the proposed theory from social conflict theory. Its use is particularly appropriate for the development of a theory of intergroup rivalry because it examines the role of conflict between groups in a social system. Social conflict theory provides a realistic picture of the competitive climate faced by practitioners because it recognizes the dynamics of group interdependence. In deriving this marketing-oriented theory of intergroup rivalry from social conflict theory, the proposed theory will address the following research questions: how does group membership influence the behavior of group members, and, what impact does the behavior of group-oriented members have on the outcomes of rivalrous interactions? A review of relevant literature follows in Chapter 2. I review the concept of rivalry and present its dimensions. Then I provide a review of group research, focusing on group identification and group cohesion. In the section entitled Theoretical Framework I explore the relevance of social conflict theory to the development of the proposed theory. Finally, the propositions and supporting text are presented in the Theory Development section. Chapter 3 explains the methodology, including sampling, measures, instrument, data collection and data analysis. Chapter 4 presents results of the analysis. A discussion of results, limitations and implications for future research follow in Chapter 5. Chapter 2 Literature Review Competitive tension exists between individual firms in a market. Such tensions are the unit of analysis in studies of individual rivalry. Competitive tensions also exist between alliance groups. This tension between groups is the unit of analysis in studies of intergroup rivalry. The merging of rivalry and group research provides a new method of examining the influence of group membership on competitive behavior. Social conflict theory effectively explains implications of group-based rivalry, which examines conflict episodes between groups in a social system. This chapter begins with reviews of rivalry and group research. In the last section of the chapter I explain the relevance of social conflict theory and develop propositions for this group-based theory of rivalry. Rivalry Rivalry has been a tOpic of study in several disciplines (see Appendix A for rivalry literature review table). Management strategists and economists define it as: firms jockeying for incompatible market positions (Porter 1980; Scherer and Ross 1990), firms jockeying for incompatible market positions and resources (Baum and Korn 1996 ), firms jockeying for patronage of buyers (Brenner 1987), and competitive interactions between specific pairs/sets of firms (Chen 1996a; Chen, Smith and Grim 1992; Smith, et al. 6 1997). Rivalry refers to the exchange of competitive moves between specific firms, which distinguishes it from competition, a general concept describing a market or industry rather than specific entities within it (Baum and Korn 1996; Chen 1996a; Chen 1996b; Chen and Hambrick 1995; Chen and MacIVfillan 1992; Chen, Smith and Grim 1992; Smith, et a1. 1997). Sociologists describe rivalry as a competitive outcome, where the goal is to minimize the absolute gains of the competitor (Knight and Kagan 1977). Social psychologists define sibling rivalry as children in a family competing for parental attention (Irish 1964). Studies of sibling rivalry suggest it to be a dimension of a relationship, which results in conflict (F urrnan and Buhrmester 1985; Rafaelli 1992). Studies of rivalry between individuals comprise the bulk of rivalry research. Strategic management studies have examined inter-firm rivalry and behavioral science studies examined sibling rivalry. Only a few studies of rivalry between groups have appeared, primarily in the strategic group literature. Group Research Sociologists derive group identification from social identity theory (Kelly and Kelly 1994), which proposes that engaging in social comparisons of out groups motivates individuals to achieve positive social identity through in group distinctiveness (Hogg, Terry and White 1995). They define group identification as the desire an individual has to connect with or work with a particular group (Kelly and Kelly 1994). Social conflict theorists propose that the likelihood of intergroup conflict increases if groups develop a sense of identity (Rubin, Pruitt and Kim 1994). 7 Group cohesion is a construct found in the psychology literature that is very similar in meaning to group identification. Psychologists define it as the attraction of a group to its members (Roether and Peters 1972), the connectedness of the group (Budman, et al. 1989) and the willingness to continue to work together (Vinokur-Kaplan 1995). Theoretical Framework W Social conflict theory explains disorder and change in society. In particular, it analyzes ”elements of inequality, power, domination and conflict in human societies" (Turner 1991, p. 183). Social conflict theory proposes that conflict is a process that helps to maintain the social system, which is composed of members vying for scarce resources (such as power or competitive advantage). Mechanisms that generate change and disorder in society, such as power differentials, scarce resources and different interest groups, cause conflict to erupt and, therefore, are critical in understanding social systems. The recent work of Thomas (1992) recognizes conflict as a basic process which organizations must manage. Structural conditions of the system influence the process, it has an internal logic, and it produces consequences for the accomplishment of tasks and the maintenance of the social system. As a basis for a theory of group-based rivalry, conflict theory has an advantage over competition theory because it studies conflicts of interest between groups within a society and recognizes the interdependence of behaviors, environment, and outcomes. Thus, conflict theory provides a unique viewpoint for the development of propositions concerning group-based competition in an industry. 8 According to a general model of conflict (Thomas 1992), structural conditions of the social system influence the conflict process, such as the characteristics of the parties, the characteristics of the relationship between the parties, and the characteristics of the environment (see Figure l). The important party characteristic in this study is the level of group identification found in an alliance group. According to the model, as a structural characteristic, the level of group identification may have an influence on the behaviors exhibited by group members in a conflict situation. The conflict process includes awareness or perception of a goal conflict between the groups and the behaviors exhibited by those groups. A goal conflict exists when the results wanted by one party are incompatible with the results wanted by the opposing party. Awareness/perception of goal conflict serves as the link between the structural influences on the potential conflict parties and the actual execution of behavior. The parties must perceive a conflict before they take action. Outcomes are the results of the conflict episode. For organizations, these results might be expressed as sales figures, profit figures or other financial data, or as desirable attributes, such as innovation or creativity. This model serves as the basis for the structural model derived from the proposed theory. Theory Development In the proposed theory, rivalry is the result of a goal conflict between a firm protecting and one challenging the current distribution of scarce resources. It is based on a win-lose outcome preference. Rivalry is the cause of two or more individuals/firms interacting to capture scarce resources to which all parties believe they are entitled, at the expense of the 9 competitor. Two distinct characteristics indicate intergroup rivalry: a ‘win-lose’ orientation and an ‘us versus them’ stance. See Figure 2 for a model of the proposed theory. W The conflict process developed by Thomas (1992) proposes that awareness triggers a conflict episode. Awareness is the realization that goal conflicts exist between groups concerning the distribution of scarce resources. Coser (1956) proposed that the existence of unequal distribution of resources does not necessarily lead to rivalry. "Before a social conflict between negatively and positively privileged groups can take place, before hostile attitudes are turned into social action, the negatively privileged group must first develop the awareness that it is, indeed, negatively privileged" (Coser 1956, p. 37). Some researchers studying rivalry between individual units have suggested that certain motives cause rivalrous behavior. A study of the airline industry (Baum and Kom 1996) identified a motive that influenced rivals to cooperate to avoid retaliation. MacCrimmon and Messick (1976), in a theoretical paper, proposed social motives for behavior based on outcomes and payoffs for self and other. Those influenced by self-interest and self- sacrifice motives consider only payoffs to self. Others influenced by altruism and aggression consider only payofi's to other. Those influenced by cooperation and competition motivations consider payoffs to self and other. Relative, rather than absolute, payoffs characterize these motives. A study of competitive actions in the U. S. airline industry identified three behavioral antecedents as positively related to interfirm rivalry: 10 awareness of interfirm relationships and action implications, motivation to act and capability to act (Chen 1996b; Miller and Chen 1994). Hare (1976) proposed that group members have many observations and facts available from which to interpret reality and to problem solve. Opinions expressed in groups are more thoughtful and prepared than opinions of individuals. Group discussion eliminates some observations. Groups tend to make fewer, more effective decisions than individuals, due to intra-group interaction. The wide knowledge range and better quality decisions make groups more aware than individuals of goal conflicts between competitors. Hartford (1971) proposed that group decisions are the result of exploration of various possibilities of alternative issues, and a consensus happens only after much interaction. The implication is that groups are more aware than individuals of potential conflict situations. Struch and Schwartz (1989) found empirical evidence of a negative relationship between group identification of religious groups and perceived conflict in the prediction of aggressive behavior. However, the authors note that strong social pressures existing at the time of the study may have confounded perceptions of perceived conflict. Based on social conflict theory and group research, I propose that group identification, a structural characteristic in the conflict process, will influence awareness of conflict. Thus, I propose: P1: The greater the level of group identification, the greater the perceived awareness of goal conflict. 1 l m Coser (1956) proposes that when the existing distribution of scarce resources in a social system is no longer considered acceptable, individuals with common interests will form groups to Signal their dissatisfaction with the status quo. Dissatisfaction is expressed through conflict behaviors. Rubin, Pruitt and Kim (1994) propose that group support affirms the legitimacy of a group’s goals, increasing the likelihood that the group will initiate conflict behaviors to attain its goals. They also note that groups protest more than individuals because the identity of individual members depends on the belief that one’s own group is superior to other groups. Hartford’s (1971) social work research led her to propose that an increased level of group cohesion is associated with an increase in member motivation to participate in group tasks. Insko et al. (1992) found empirical evidence that groups of students competing in Prisoner’s Dilemma type games were more competitive than individuals, and that groups became more aggressive in their competitive behaviors as individual members began to identify with their group. In an experiment with schoolchildren, perceived similarity of competing groups increased intergroup rivalry in a competitive situation as the groups attempted to maintain their uniqueness and superiority (Brown 1984). Group identification was a significant predictor of participatory behavior of union members (Kelly and Kelly 1994). The above research suggests a direct, positive influence of group identification on behavior. P2: The greater the level of group identification, the greater the use of rivalrous behaviors. 12 Thomas (1992) proposes that structural factors, such as characteristics of the parties, characteristics of the environment, and characteristics of the relationship between conflict parties influence the conflict process. Empirical studies show that these factors influence behavior. Researchers proposed that numerous characteristics influence rivalry between individual units. Empirical evidence in airline industry studies suggests that past performance, experience breadth, and age of organization relate positively to the range of rivalrous behaviors used (Miller and Chen 1996). In addition, firm size is a significant indicator of a firrn’s propensity to initiate competitive actions and firm propensity to respond to competitive attacks (Chen and Hambrick 1995). Retailers use technology and market criteria to develop cognitive categorizations defining competitive relationships between rivals, with managers defining rivals as only those stores most similar to their own organization in terms of self-defined technology and market factors (Porac and Thomas 1994). In the child development literature, children reported greater rivalry with younger than older siblings. Also, feelings of rivalry were greater with wide-spaced younger siblings in large families (Furman and Buhrmester 1985). Older children (twelfth graders) reported less rivalry with older siblings than did younger children (third graders) (Buhrmester and Furrnan 1990). Characteristics of the individual units also influence group based rivalry. The group to which participants belonged influenced rivalry between strategic groups. Behaviors between members of the same strategic group were less competitive than behaviors between members of different strategic groups (Peteraf 1993). 13 Studies concerning the influence of environmental characteristics on rivalry all examined rivalry between individual units. Market diversity, breadth of customers and competitors in a market, and market growth are inversely related to the range of rivalrous behaviors initiated (Miller and Chen 1996). Market concentration relates to the incidence of rivalrous behaviors in a U-shaped curve; when concentration is low, more firms enter the market by initiating rivalrous behaviors, as concentration increases fewer firms enter the market and market concentration drops (Baum and Korn 1996). Brahm (1995) proposes that aggressive national targeting policies promote overinvestment in high technology industries, which increases rivalrous interactions between firms. Characteristics of the relationship between competitors also influence rivalry. Interdependence, defined in the management literatures as an overlap of customers, markets and/or resources, is associated with a decrease in the initiation of rivalrous behaviors between individual units (Baum and Kom 1996; Chen 1996b; Porac and Thomas 1994; Porter 1980) and between strategic groups (Mehra 1996). Absolute deprivation does not always result in conflict behaviors; it is the awareness of relative positions that leads groups to initiate conflict behaviors (Turner 1991). Coser (1956) proposes that when groups compare positions, the negatively privileged perceive a position of relative deprivation and emulate and seek the resources possessed by groups they consider to be relatively more privileged. In the attempt to gain the coveted resources of the positively privileged groups, the less privileged initiate conflict behaviors. Struch and Schwartz (1989) found a positive relationship between group identification and 14 aggressive behavior, however, they noted an indirect link between the two constructs. Their study of religious group conflict showed that perceived awareness of conflict moderated the impact of group identification on group behavior. Thus, I propose: P3: Perceived goal conflict results in a greater use of rivalrous behaviors in the presence of strong group identification. 92mg. The conflict process of Thomas (1992) proposes that behaviors influence outcomes. Studies of rivalry between individual units confirm Thomas, that characteristics of actions/behaviors influence outcomes of rivalry. Failure to follow typical industry behavior harms performance. Positive or negative deviations from group norms of action execution speed hurt small airline performance and deviations from group norms of propensity to initiate actions and response announcement speed hurt large airline performance (Chen and Hambrick 1995). Other research on the airline industry by Chen and his colleagues exposes numerous behavioral characteristics. Visibility and centrality of rivalrous behaviors relate positively to the likelihood of retaliation while difficulty of implementing response behaviors relates inversely to the likelihood of retaliation. The authors suggest that avoiding retaliation increases success (Chen and Miller 1994). In the airline industry, a narrow range of competitive activities results in poor performance in periods of uncertainty and market grth (Miller and Chen 1996). The number of competitors directly afi‘ected by an attack and the degree that an action threatens a firm’s key markets relate positively to the number of responses provoked by an attack. The effort required to implement an attack relates inversely to the number of responses 15 provoked by the action, the more diflicult the implementation, the fewer the responses to that action. Strategic actions have a negative influence on the number of competitive response behaviors and tactical actions have a positive influence on the number of competitive responses (Chen, Smith and Grim 1992). Characteristics of responses, such as number of responses and length of response lag, influence outcomes for the initiating and responding firms. The greater the number of responses an action provokes, the lower the performance of the initiating firm (Chen, Smith and Grimm 1992). The greater the lag between an action and a response, the greater the performance of the responding firm. This unusual finding suggests that the time spent on developing an effective response is productive (Smith, Grimm, Gannon and Chen 1991). Studies examining rivalry between individual firms found that intense and extended rivalry negatively influences rate of return (Brahm 1995), risk-retum outcomes (Cool, Dierickx and Jemison 1989), and return on assets and sales (Schul, Davis and Hartline 1995). Intensity of rivalrous behaviors is associated with negative outcomes (Baum and Korn 1996). Excessive rivalry between college athletes and a sibling was found to result in poor athletic performance (Hanna 1993). Irish (1964) suggests that sibling rivalry has dysfirnctional effects on the socialization of children. It disrupts and hinders the child’s development of the ability to function as a member of the family and society. 16 The social conflict theory of Coser (1956) proposes that conflict between groups provides functional benefits for the groups and for the social system. In contrast, rivalry between strategic groups in the pharmaceutical industry negatively afl’ects industry and firm profits (Cool and Dierickx 1993). Group cohesion is a psychological concept that is equivalent to the sociological concept of group identification. Empirical results of early group research Show that cohesiveness relates positively to productivity, that more cohesive groups are more productive (Hare 1976). Several more recent studies support the early research. Budman et al. (1989) provided empirical evidence that the more cohesive the psychotherapy group an individual patient participated in, the greater the patient’s improvement in symptoms. However, the researchers noted that group process played an important role in the outcome; highly cohesive groups exhibited more effective group therapy behaviors than less cohesive groups, resulting in more positive outcomes. A strong positive relationship between group cohesion and effectiveness of mental health teams was found, and group cohesion and efl’ective outcomes were dependent on the collaborative behaviors of group members (Vinokur-Kaplan 1995). A study of female, collegiate golfers found that the level of cohesion of the golf team related positively to performance (Williams and Widmeyer 1991). This study also noted a link with behavior, specifically, that more cohesive teams demonstrated better communication and coordination skills during their golf matches. A study of sexual offenders found those participating in cohesive groups were more likely than those in less cohesive groups to experience treatment failure (Roether and Peters 17 1972). Hostile behavior during group therapy, rather than group cohesiveness, was associated with treatment success for offenders. McGrath also found a negative cohesion- performance relationship (1962) in a study of rifle teams. Since most research shows a positive relationship between group identification and performance, and notes behavior as a direct antecedent to outcome, I propose: P4: Rivalrous behavior produces positive outcomes in the presence of strong group identification. In summary, the proposed theory suggests that the level of identification alliance group members feel toward their group is an important stnrctural factor that influences rivalry with other groups. Strong feelings of group identification heighten awareness of goal conflicts with rival groups and increase the use of rivalrous behaviors. Heightened awareness of goal conflicts also directly increases the use of rivalrous behavior. Following the conflict theory of Lewis Coser (1956; 1967), I propose that increased use of rivalrous behaviors produces positive outcomes for the alliance groups. The methodology used to test this theory is presented in the next chapter. Chapter 3 Methodology Measures Group Identification Group identification measures the connectedness a group member feels toward the retail cooperative group of affiliation, in comparison to other retail cooperative groups. This construct is a ‘characteristic of the parties’ in the general conflict model. I used a scale adapted for the marketing literature fi'om social identity theory that measured group identification of art museum members (Bhattacharya, Rao and Glynn 1995). Respondents in the cited study answered the six scale questions on a five-point Likert-type scale, and the authors reported a Cronbach’s alpha of 0.87. For the current study, the questions were adapted for use with cooperative groups by changing the word ‘museum’ to ‘cooperative group.’ This study used a five-point Likert-type scale (1=strongly disagree, 3=neutral, 5=strongly agree). See Table 1 for item content. Goal Conflict Goal conflict measures the extent to which a group member perceives a conflict between his or her unit goals and the goals of his or her primary retail cooperative competitor. Conflicting goals mean one party will achieve their goals only at the expense of the other party. The perception of goal conflict is the precursor to behavior. Therefore it is a 18 19 critical link between the level of group identification that a party feels and the behaviors that result in a goal conflict situation. To measure this construct, I used a measure of intergroup conflict developed by Rahim (1983; Rahim and Psenicka 1995). Respondents indicated their level of agreement with six questions concerning conflicting versus cooperative relationships with their primary retail cooperative competitor. Respondents provided answers to the Likert-type questions on a five-point scale (l=strongly disagree, 3=neutral, 5=strongly agree). Questions 1, 3 and 5 were reverse scored. Rahim reported an alpha reliability of .79 for this scale. See Table 2 for question content. Beam A scale from the entrepreneurial literature covering a broad range of competitive behaviors typical of general business strategies comprises the behavior section of the instrument (Covin 1991). The original scale asked respondents for their level of agreement to twenty, five-point Likert-type questions (1=strongly disagree, 3=neutral, 5=strongly agree) concerning their competitive behaviors. Covin calculated separate reliabilities for several sub-scales within the overall group of behaviors. Coeflicient alphas by sub-scale include external financing = .69, advertising = .84, product quality = .79, operating efficiency = .73, external independence = .66, and industry awareness = .80. Covin used the remaining questions as single-item measures. Craig, Martin and Honidge (1997) recently used this scale and calculated an alpha of .76 for the entire twenty questions. Their exploratory factor analysis showed the scale is composed of six behavioral factors corresponding to advertising, quality, forecasting, cost, financing and service strategies. Pretests for the current study revealed that three questions (minimize 20 dependence on single supplier, minimize dependence on single customer, and ofi’er superior warranties) from the original twenty were inappropriate for the population due to the unique nature of the retail hardware cooperative business. I deleted these three questions on the final instrument. Only one question of Craig et. al’s (1997) cost factor remained after the deletion. Based on Covin’s factors, I expected this question to load on the quality factor, resulting in five behavioral factors, rather than six. The subtractions resulted in a scale composed of seventeen questions. I asked that respondents indicate the importance of each behavior in competing against their most important cooperative- afliliated competitor on a five-point scale (1=not at all important, 3=moderately important, 5=extremely important). See Table 3 for question content. Behavior is treated as a second-order factor in the analysis, meaning it is composed of several first-order factors. These first-order factors correspond to the specific types of competitive behaviors contained in this scale. Outcome Relative performance, in terms of effectiveness (sales growth) and efliciency (profitability), was the measure for outcome and was captured by a three-item scale fi'om the retailing literature (Smith, Venkatraman and Wortzel 1995). The authors of the scale asked respondents how their sales and profit results compared with competitors on three, five-point Likert-type questions (l=much below competition, 3=same as competition, 5=much better than competition). Pretests for the current study found that retailers were unable to assess their results in comparison to competitors. They were, however, aware of industry averages. So, respondents compared sales and profit results of their store to 21 industry average (l=much below industry average, 3=same as industry average, 5=much above industry average). The authors of the scale reported a coefficient alpha of .83. See Table 4 for question content. Sample The sample for this study was chosen from the set of retail firms employing partnering strategies. Retail buying cooperatives are a common partnering strategy (Maynard 1996). Buying cooperatives are attractive because they allow small retailers to increase operating efficiency, gain bargaining power with suppliers, and enhance market presence with consumers (Ghosh 1994, Levvison 1994). The study sample consists of members of retail buying cooperatives in the home improvement industry. This industry is attractive for a study of intergroup rivalry for two reasons. The first reason is the recent competitive conditions experienced by industry members. Since 1982, concentration in the home improvement industry has shifted from small retailers to large retailers (U .S. Department of Commerce 1995, 1990, 1985). For example, in 1982, the four largest building material and garden centers controlled 7.1% of the market; in 1992, they controlled 16%. The four largest hardware stores increased their share of the market from 7.9% in 1982 to 9.7% in 1992. The National Retail Hardware Association estimated the top twenty-five home-improvement chains controlled more than 35% of the market in 1995 (Quintanifla 1996). This industry has experienced a highly competitive environment in the 1990s. Hardware cooperatives are competing with national hardware chains, such as Home Depot and Lowe’s, and with other retail 22 hardware cooperatives (Quintanilla 1996, Schulz 1996b, 1996c, Stankevich 1996). The hardware industry is also a good choice for this study because several retail cooperatives have national representation. A national sample avoids bias that may result from unusual regional conditions, making the results of the study generalizable to a larger population. The member list of the National Retail Hardware Association (NRHA) comprised the sampling fi'ame. This list contains 60,300 contact names fi'om 26,200 retailers and NRHA updates and audits the list monthly. NRHA includes members of all the national retail hardware cooperatives, ensuring variability in the independent variables in the study. I purchased a sample of 1700 names from the NRHA. They selected every fifteenth store fiom their list in a systematic random sampling routine. They included only names of owners or presidents on the purchased list and they omitted duplicate contact names in the sampling routine. Data Collection I developed and pretested a three-page questionnaire on a small group of twelve member owners of retail hardware cooperative stores. A pretest of twelve to twenty-five cases is generally sufficient to reveal problems with an instrument (Rossi, Wright and Anderson 1983). I telephoned representative retail hardware cooperative owners to solicit participation in the pretest, mailed a questionnaire to those agreeing to participate, and made an appointment for pick up. I debriefed respondents at the pick up appointment, noted actual respondent completion times and questionnaire problems and made adjustments accordingly. 23 I mailed the final instrument to a random sample of 1599 member owners (Appendix B). From the list of 1700 purchased from NRHA, I deleted seven store owners who had been contacted for the pretest. I also deleted 94 names fi'om stores with multiple owner/executive contacts. For stores with multiple contacts, only the first name appearing on the list received a questionnaire. A personalized letter from the researcher accompanied questionnaires suggesting the relevance of the study to the participants. I also invited respondents to request results of the survey. Approximately ten percent of respondents did so. From the initial mailing of 1599, I received 377 responses within four weeks. Six surveys were returned as undeliverable and were deducted from the quantity initially mailed and not replaced. A total of 377 received from a total of 1593 distributed resulted in a total response rate of 24%. Over half of the stores (52%) in the sample have been in operation twenty-five years or less. Respondents tended to operate only one store (82%), with five or fewer full-time employees (44%), under 10,000 square feet (69%), with sales volume under one million dollars in 1996 (49%). See Table 5 for sample characteristics. The NRHA was unable to segment its list into cooperative versus non-cooperative members. It also does not maintain records about the type of store each member operates. All of the major retail hardware cooperatives have several different types of stores that sell hardware, the most usual types being hardware stores, lumberyards, and home centers. The three types of stores differ in annual sales volumes, square footage, number of employees, and customer base (retail end-user versus contractor customers). Because it is 24 unknown whether owners of different types of stores differ on the key constructs in the study, the instrument contained questions to subdivide respondents into the proper segments for analysis. The questionnaire also contained several questions to discover the competitive situation of the respondent, to ensure that they were eligible to participate. Based on the screening questions, I grouped respondents into categories and analyzed only hardware stores, who were members of a retail hardware cooperative, and recognized a cooperative-afliliated competitor from a different cooperative group. Out of 377 respondents, 38 (10%) were not members of a cooperative, 93 (25%) were members of a non-hardware cooperative group, 67 (18%) did not recognize a competitor belonging to a retail buying cooperative, and 32 (8%) cited a member of their own cooperative group as the most important competitor (intra-group competition). Subtracting all of the above as not eligible for the current study results in a usable sample size of 147 hardware retailers who are affiliated with a retail hardware cooperative and recognize as an important competitor a hardware retailer who is a member of a different hardware cooperative. Representativeness of the usable sample was checked using a one-sample t-test on average sales volume, average square footage and average number of employees. The one-sample t-test tests the null hypothesis that there is no difference between the mean of a population and a known test value. The NRHA provided industry averages for these three variables for all three types of stores. I used the NRHA figures for hardware stores as the known test values. Using .05 as the cutofi’, the t-tests show that the 147 hardware stores in this sample were not significantly different from the average U. S. hardware store on any of the three variables. See Table 6 for results. 25 A test for non-response bias was performed on the 147 hardware stores using independent sample t-tests to probe for differences between early and late respondents. Comparing the means of the summed scores of each of the key constructs in the study tested the null hypothesis that the scores of early respondents are equal to the scores of late respondents. T-tests Show no significant difference between early and late respondents on any of the constructs. See Table 7 for t-test results. Data Analysis I tested the proposed theory with a structural equation model. Following the two-step approach recommended by Anderson and Gerbing (1988), I estimated the measurement model first and then continued with the structural model. To assess the measurement model, I performed a confirmatory factor analysis on the first order factors, group identification, awareness of goal conflict, quality behavior, financing behavior, forecasting behavior, advertising behavior, customer service behavior, and performance outcome (see Figure 3). I used the covariance matrix of the scale items as input for the measurement model. Chapter 4 describes estimation and adjustment of the measurement model. The covariances of the factors (phi matrix) produced by the revised measurement model served as the input for the second stage of the analysis, the structural model. The final model is presented in Figure 4. Chapter 4 explains the results of the data analysis. Chapter 4 Results The first section of this chapter explains the confirmatory analysis performed on the first order factors in the measurement model. A discussion of the structural model follows and the chapter closes with the results of the hypothesis testing. Figure 3 shows the initial measurement model that was subjected to confirmatory analysis. Figure 4 shows the final measurement model and the structural equation model tested in this analysis. I used generalized least squares estimation in the EQS program (Version 3.00) to fit the model. Measurement Model I began the process by estimating a confirmatory factor analysis to test the hypothesized relationships of the thirty-one observed variables to the nine latent constructs. Eight first- order constructs were specified with unit variances, as recommended by Gerbing and Hunter (1982). These constructs were modeled as a six-item group identification factor (F 1: Group Id), a five-item awareness of goal conflict factor (F 2: Goal Conflict), a five- item quality behavior factor (F3: Quality), a two-item financing behavior factor (F4: Financing), a three-item forecasting behavior factor (F5: Forecasting), a four-item advertising behavior factor (F6: Advertising), a three-item customer service factor (F7 : Customer Service), and a three-item performance outcome factor (F9: Outcome). This 26 27 model was under identified. To revise the model, I examined the pattern of standardized residuals and the Lagrange multiplier tests (the equivalent of the chi-square difference test), and deleted two variables from the behavior scale items that loaded onto multiple factors. The revised model produced a solution. However, the Lagrange multiplier test indicated many problems with this measurement model. The Lagrange multiplier test evaluates the effect of adding parameters to a model. For the model under consideration, the Lagrange multiplier test showed that several indicators loaded onto more than one construct with statistical significance. Anderson and Gerbing (1988) note the importance in structural equation modeling of unidirnensional constructs and propose that deleting indicators from a model is preferable to relating one indicator to multiple factors because interpretation is diflicult when indicators are assigned to multiple factors. Therefore, I deleted variables with significant multivariate probability in the Lagrange multiplier test from the model sequentially until I achieved a parsimonious model. Deletion of eight behavioral indicators resulted in the deletion of three first-order behavior factors. This left three first-order behavioral factors, quality, control, and advertising, loading onto behavior, the second- order factor. The final model contains nineteen observed variables and seven underlying constructs, one of which is the second-order factor (see Figure 4). I assessed the measurement of each construct in the model for internal consistency. Measures of composite reliability, Cronbach’s alpha, and variance extracted provide evidence of internal consistency. See Appendix C for formulas used to calculate 28 composite reliability and variance extracted. Table 8 presents these figures for the revised measurement model. All constructs exceed the .7 recommendation for composite reliability and Cronbach’s coefficient alpha, providing evidence of internal consistency. Measures of variance extracted, which shows the amount of variance of the indicators accounted for by the latent construct, exceed the recommended benchmark of .5 for all but two constructs. Group identification and awareness of goal conflict show variance extracted measures of .43 and .46 respectively. These two constructs are derived from the sociology literatures and the low variance extracted may indicate a need for firrther measure refinement. Evidence of convergent validity is provided when the coefficient of each indicator (variables with a V label in Figure 4) to its construct is significant. The parameter estimate for every indicator in the tested measurement model is significant (see Figure 4 and Table 8). Table 8 also contains the overall results of the revised measurement model. The chi- square value for the model is 160.553, based on 137 degrees of fi'eedom, with a probability of 0.083, indicating a good fit. The chi-square value is an absolute measure of fit and can be sensitive to sample size and number of indicators in the model. The number of indicators in this model is high, so fit indices other than the chi-square should be examined. Incremental fit measures compare the fit of a tested model to a null baseline model. EQS provides two incremental fit measures. The first, the Bentler-Bonett Normed Fit Index (NFI) is .982 for the model. The second, the Bentler-Bonett Nonnormed Fit Index (NNF I), takes into account the degrees of freedom in a model. This index is .997. Both of the incremental fit indices indicate a good fit of the data to the 29 measurement model. Parsimonious fit measures are more precise than absolute fit and incremental fit indices because they evaluate the fit of a model in relationship to its degrees of freedom and sample size. The parsimonious fit index calculated in the EQS program is the Comparative Fit Index (CPI). The Comparative Fit Index for the proposed measurement model is .997 . The various fit indices generally support the revised measurement model. Structural Model Estimation of the structural parameters is the second step in the Structural equation modeling technique recommended by Anderson and Gerbing (1988). The results of this step are presented in Table 9. The overall fit of the model is adequate, with chi-square of 12.414, 8 degrees of freedom, and p = .134. The Bentler-Bonett Normed Fit Index (NFI) is .990, the Bentler-Bonett Nonnormed Fit Index (NNF I) is .993, and the Comparative Fit Index (CFI) is .996. Behavior is a second-order factor in the structural model and the three first-order factors (quality, control and advertising) loading onto behavior were all significant. The loadings are included in Figure 4 and Table 9. Hypotheses Examination of the parameter estimates shows support for most of the linkages in the proposed theory (see Table 9). I proposed a positive relationship between level of group identification and awareness of goal conflict in Proposition 1. The path between F2 (goal conflict) and F1 (group identification) represents this relationship in Figure 4. Results show a significant positive relationship between group identification and awareness of goal 30 conflict (F 2 Fl = .146; p < .05). This suggests that the more a retail cooperative member identifies with his or her group of affiliation, the more likely he or she is to perceive a goal conflict with a rival. Such a finding is consistent with Hare’s (1976) proposition that groups are more aware than individuals of goal conflicts between rivals and with Hartford’s (1971) proposition that groups are more aware of potential conflict (rivalrous) situations. Proposition 2 proposed a positive relationship between the level of group identification and the use of rivalrous behaviors. The path between F8 (behavior) and F1 (group identification) represents this relationship in Figure 4. The significant F8 F1 link supports this proposition (F8 F 1 = .345; p < .01), implying that the more a cooperative member identifies with the group, the greater the use of rivalrous behaviors. This finding is consistent with propositions and empirical findings in the sociology literature. Rubin, Pruitt and Kim (1994) proposed that group support increases goal-oriented group behaviors. Hartford (1971) proposed increased levels of group cohesion to be associated with increased motivation to participate in group tasks. Studies by Insko et al. (1992) and Brown (1984) found that intergroup rivalry in student groups increased as the groups developed identities. Findings in the current study also support those of Kelly and Kelly (1994), who found that group identification was a significant predictor of participatory behavior of union members. I proposed a positive relationship between awareness of goal conflict and the use of rivalrous behaviors in Proposition 3 and the path between goal conflict (F2) and behavior 31 (F 8) represents this relationship. The link between F8 and F2 is not significant. This finding suggests that a group member’s awareness of goal conflict with a rival does not influence the use of rivalrous behaviors. This is contrary to the findings of Struch and Schwartz (1989), who found that the greater a group member’s awareness of goal conflict, the more aggressive the behaviors used. Perhaps this suggests a missing link in the conflict process tested in the model. Perceived goal conflict may not directly cause rivalrous behaviors, but may instead influence motives, which directly cause behavior. A few respondents noted that their behaviors derive from their motives toward either their customers or their competitors. The last proposition proposed a positive relationship between rivalrous behavior of group members and performance outcomes. A non-significant link between F9 (outcome) and F8 (behavior) in Figure 3 does not support this proposition. This implies that behaviors arising from feelings of group identification and awareness of goal conflicts with rivals do not affect financial performance of group members. This finding is inconsistent both with management literature, which generally find a negative relationship between rivalrous behaviors and financial performance (Cool and Dierickx 1993; Brahm 1995; Cool, Dierickx and Jemison 1989; Schul, Davis and Hartline 1995), and sociology literature, which propose a positive relationship between rivalrous behavior and performance (Williams and Widmeyer 1991; Vinokur-Kaplan 1995; Budman et al. 1989). Such a finding may suggest that financial performance is not an appropriate measure for behavioral results. In such a competitive market, retailers may have competed profits away, making other outcome measures, such as customer satisfaction or innovation more 32 appropriate. Overall, the results of the current study show that group identification relates positively to awareness of goal conflict and the use of rivalrous behaviors. This infers that higher levels of group identification increase the awareness of goal conflict with a rival and the use of rivalrous behaviors. Awareness of goal conflict is not significantly related to the use of rivalrous behaviors. The study did not find a significant relationship between the use of rivalrous behaviors and financial outcomes. I discuss these results in Chapter 5. Chapter 5 Conclusion The current study set out to examine characteristics of a type of competition gaining prominence in the marketplace, competition between groups. Although group-based methods of competing have been a part of the retail industry for many years, we should not underestimate the effect of their recent gain in popularity. From the sample responding to this study, 18% (67) of the total sample, and 28% (39) of the hardware stores answered that their number one competitor was a member of another retail cooperative group. While the majority of the respondents considered inter-type competition, for example competition between cooperatives and Big Box retailers, the biggest threat, the number of retailers considering group based competition to be a threat was noteworthy. \Vrth more than 44,000 home improvement outlets operating in the US. in 1996, and over 20,000 stores belonging to wholesaling cooperatives, the importance to the retail industry of this rising form of competition becomes clear (“The Industry’s Annual Report,” 1996). It is critical that hardware retailers understand the dynamics of group-based behavior to survive in a competitive environment where they must compete on two fronts, against category killers and group-based retailers. 33 34 I developed the proposed theory to explain how group identification influences the competitive behaviors of group members. Group identification is a feeling of connectedness that a member feels with other members of the group. Cooperative management and fellow members can foster this feeling. Interaction of members is a key factor in the development of group identification. This theory proposed that group member awareness of goal conflicts with rivals would be more acute because of the interaction between group members. The analysis supported this proposition. A second proposition of the theory is that feelings of solidarity and support among group members inspire action, which results in increased rivalrous behaviors in groups with higher levels of group identification. The analysis also supported this proposition. Contrary to expectations, the study found that increased awareness of goal conflict did not influence the incidence of rivalrous behaviors, perhaps indicating that another construct, such as motives, directly influences rivalrous behavior. The last finding of the study, that higher levels of rivalrous behaviors do not relate to financial performance, suggests that other types of outcomes may be more important in a highly competitive environment. This study has provided some useful empirical evidence about group-based competition in the retail hardware industry upon which to build future research. Implications The influence of group membership on individual behavior raises some important implications for this theory. Being a member of a retail cooperative will influence the behavior of the independent owner. Owners expect certain rights as independents, but also take on responsibilities and face behavioral norms as members of a cooperative. 35 Member owners and cooperative management need to be aware of and understand this influence to operate effectively. Group identification should not be confirsed with conformity. Social conflict theory warns of the dangers of high levels of forced conformity to group goals and ideals. Such conformity may lead to within-group conflict, ultimately resulting in a split of the group into rival factions. So, while I advise that cooperative groups foster feelings of group identification within the group, the goal is to increase the interaction of group members, not the conformity. An implication that is sometimes considered counterintuitive is that cooperative groups should encourage inter-group rivalry, rather than discourage it. Rivalry fosters innovation and creativity of a group. If not carried to excess, increased inter-group rivalry may improve operating results. Limitations and Future Research Several limitations of this theory exist. The generalizability of the study results is one limitation. The findings may not apply to types of alliance groups other than retail cooperatives. Retail cooperatives may possess unique levels of group identification or exhibit behaviors peculiar to their industry. Also, theoretical application may be limited to the retail industry. Retailers face a distinct set of marketing mix decisions, which may result in behaviors not applicable to other industries. Hardware retailers may currently be facing a unique situation within the retail environment, making their situation unique in 36 comparison to retailers competing in other lines of business. Thus, we must undertake carefirl consideration before generalizing the results of this study to populations other than retail hardware cooperatives. This theory proposes relationships between sociology-based constructs applied to a retail- oriented business setting. To test the model, I adapted measures for several proposed constructs from the sociology literature. Although I pretested these scales on a small sample, refinements are still needed, as evidenced by the reliability scores and variance extracted of the adapted scales. Future research efforts should be made to refine the scales used in this study or develop new ones. This theory proposes a dynamic process. Therefore, longitudinal measurement may add more to our understanding of inter-group rivalry than one-time measurements. Future replication projects are encouraged to learn more about the dynamic nature of the influence of group membership on competitive retail behaviors and outcomes. Antecedents to group identification may be a salient research topic. Alliance groups in the business community need to know what increases the identification of members to the group. Such knowledge can help groups attract and retain members, and increase the effectiveness of the group. This is another area where we must adapt measures from sociology to a business application. 37 The sociology literature suggests that level of group identification is inversely related to group Size, as membership increases, levels of group identification suffer. Since level of group identification affects the perceptions of group members and the behaviors of group members, cooperative management may be able to manipulate member identification with the group. Cooperative group managements may be interested in learning more about the effect of size on level of group identification as this form of competition continues to increase in popularity and as groups merge to form larger groups. Survey respondents suggested an additional area for firture research in notes on the questionnaires. Some commented that their behaviors were driven not by their competitors but by their customers. This suggests that the motives of retailers may differ, with some concentrating primarily on their customers and others concentrating on their competitors. Such a notion is consistent with the social conflict and sociology literatures reviewed. However, before motive can be added to the model, researchers must develop a measure of motive. The findings of this study show that group identification is an important sociological concept that applies to the business setting. With the tremendous rise in the use of alliance groups in the current economy, an understanding of factors influencing the behavior of group members is critical to the success of the alliance effort. I have shown that group identification is one factor that influences the behavior of group members. While this study has answered a few questions about the effect of group membership on behavior, it has presented new questions that may serve as fi'uitfirl research projects in the future. References References Anderson, J .C. & Gerbing, D.W. (1988). Structural equation modeling in practice: A review and recommended two-step approach. Psychological Bulletin. 103 (3), 411-423. Baum, J. A. C. & Korn, H. J. (1996). Competitive dynamics of interfirm rivalry. Academy of Management Journal, 32 (2), 25 5-291. Bentler, P. M. (1993). EOS Structural Equations Program Manual. Los Angeles, California: BMDP Statistical Software. Bhattacharya, C.B., Rao, H. & Glynn, MA. (1995). Understanding the bond of identification: An investigation of its correlates among art museum members. Journal of Marketing, 52 (4), 46-57. Brahm, R. (1995). National targeting policies, high-technology industries, and excessive competition. Strategic Management Journal, 16, 71-91. Brenner, R. (1987). Rivalry: In Business, Science, Among Nations. New York: Cambridge University Press. Brown, R. J. (1984). The efl‘ects of intergroup similarity and cooperative vs. competitive orientation on intergroup discrimination. British Journ_al of Social chhology, Q (1), 21- 33. Budman, S. H., Soldz, S., Demby, A., Feldstein, M., Springer, T. and Davis, M. S. (1989). Cohesion, alliance and outcome in group psychotherapy. Psychiatry, 2 (3), 339- 350. Buhrmester, D. & Furrnan, W. (1990). Perceptions of sibling relationships during middle childhood and adolescence. Child Development, 61 (5), 1387-1398. Chen, M-J. (1996a). Competitor analysis and interfirm rivalry: Toward a theoretical integration. Academy of Management Review. 21 (1), 100-134. 38 39 Chen, M-J. (1996b). Competitor analysis and inter-firm rivalry: Toward a theoretical integration. In D. P. Moore (Ed), Academy of Management Best Papers Proceedings 1995 (pp. 7-11). --- & Hambrick. D. C. (1995). Speed, stealth, and selective attack: How small firms differ from large firms in competitive behavior. Academy of Management Journal, 3 (2), 453-482. ---- & MacMillan, I. C. (1992). Nonresponse and delayed response to competitive moves: The roles of competitor dependence and action irreversibility. Academy of Management Journal, 3_5_ (3), 539-570. ---, Smith, K. G. & Grimm, C. M. (1992). Action characteristics as predictors of competitive responses. Management Science. Q (3), 43 9-455. Cool, K. & Dierickx, I. (1993). Rivalry, strategic groups and firm profitability. Strategic Mgagement qumal, L1, 47-59. ------ - & Jemison, D. (1989). Business strategy, market structure and risk-retum 2 relationships: A structural approach. Strategic Management Journal, 10 (6), 507-522. Coser, L. A. (1967). Contin_uities in the Study of Soci_al Conflict. New York: The Free Press. ---- (1956). The Functions of SociaL Conflict. New York: The Free Press. Covin, J. G. (1991). Entrepreneurial versus conservative firms: A comparison of strategies and performance. Journal of Management Studies, 28 (5), 439-462. Craig, J. S., Martin, R. & Horridge, P. (1997). Apparel manufacturing business owners: A gender comparison. Clothing_and Textiles Research Joumfi, 15 (1), 1-11. Furman, W. & Buhrmester, D. (1985). Children’s perceptions of the qualities of sibling relationships. Child Development. 56(2), 448-461. Ghosh, A. (1994). Retail Management. Secon_d Edition. Fort Worth, Texas: Dryden Press. Gomes-Casseres, B. (1994). Group versus group: How alliance networks compete. Harvard Business Review. (July-August), 62-74. Hair, J .F., Jr., Anderson, R.E., Tatharn, KL. and Black, WC. (1995). Multivariate Data Analysis with Readings, Fourth Edition. Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey: Prentice Hall. 40 Hanna, E. A. (1993). The psychodynamically oriented clinical social worker as sports consultant: A preliminary report. Clinical Social Work Joumal, 21 (3), 283-300. Hare, A. P. (1976). Handbook of Small Group Research, Second Edition. New York: The Free Press. Hartford, M. E. (1971). Groups in Soci_al Work: Application of Small Group Theory and Research to Social Work Practice. New York: Columbia University Press. Hogg, M. A., Terry, D. J. and White, K. M. (1995). A tale of two theories: A critical comparison of identity theory with social identity theory. Social Psychology Quarterly, 3 (4), 255-269. Insko, C. A., Schopler, 1., Kennedy, J. F., Dahl, K. R., Graetz, K. A. and Drigotas, S. M. (1992). Individual-group discontinuity from the difl‘ering perspectives of Campbell’s realistic group conflict theory and Tajfel and Tumer’s social identity theory. Social P cholo arterl , 5; (3), 272-291. Irish, D. P. (1964). Sibling interaction: A neglected aspect in family life research. Social Forcea, 12 (3), 280-288. Johnson, J. L. & Mendelson, S. (1996). A manifest destiny? Discount Merchandiser, (September), 26-31. Kelly, C. & Kelly, J. (1994). Who gets involved in collective action?: Social psychological determinants of individual participation in trade unions. Human Relations, 12 (1), 63-88. Knight, G. P. & Kagan, S. (1977). Development of prosocial and competitive behaviors in Anglo-American and Mexican-American children. Child Development. 18 (4), 1385- 1394. Lewison, D. M. (1994). Retailing, Fifth Edition. New York: Macmillan College Publishing Company. MacCrimmon, K. R. & Messick, D. M. (1976). A fi'amework for social motives. Behavioral Science. 2_1 (2), 86-100. Maynard, R. (1996). Striking the right match. Nation’s Business, 83 (May), 18-28. McGrath, J. E. (1962). The influence of positive interpersonal relations on adjustment and effectiveness in rifle teams. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 65 (6), 363- 37 5. 41 Mehra, A. (1996). Resource and market based determinants of performance in the US. banking industry. Strategic Management Journal, 11, 307-322. Miller, D. & Chen, M-J. (1994). Sources and consequences of competitive inertia: A study of the U. S. airline industry. Administrative Science Quarterly, 39 (1), 1-23. --- & ---. (1996). The simplicity of competitive repertoires: An empirical analysis. Strategic Management Journal, 17, 419-439. Peteraf, M. A. (1993). Intra-industry structure and the response toward rivals. Managerial and Decision Economics. fl, (6), 519-528. Porac, J. F. & Thomas, H. (1994). Cognitive categorization and subjective rivalry among retailers in a small city. Journal of Applied Psychology, 79 (1), 54-66. Porter, M. E. (1980). Competitive Strategy: Techniques for Analyzing Industries and Competitors. New York: The Free Press. Quintanilla, C. (1996, December 11). Hardware stores try to rattle big chains. Wall W. B 1. Raffaelli, M. (1992). Sibling conflict in early adolescence. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 53 (3), 652-663. Rahim, M. A. (1983). Measurement of organizational conflict. The Journal of General Paychology, 192 (October), 189-199. Rahim, M. A. & Psenicka, C. (1995). Convergent and discriminant validity of the Rahim organizational conflict inventory. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 81 (3), 1075-1078. Roether, H. A. and Peters, J. J. (1972). Cohesiveness and hostility in group psychotherapy. American Journal of Psychiatry, fl (February), 1014-1017. Rossi, P. H., Wright, J. D. & Anderson, A. B. (Eds). (1983). Handbook of Survey Research. New York: Academic Press. Rubin, J. 2., Pruitt, D. G. & Kim, S. H. (1994). Social Conflict: Escaiation. Stalemate and Settlement. New York: McGraw Hill, Inc. Scherer, F. M. & Ross, S. (1990). Industrial Market Structure aad Economic Performance. Boston: Houghton Mifllin. Schul, P. L., Davis, P. S. & Hartline, M. D. (1995). Strategic adaptation to extended rivalry: Effects on organizational performance. Journal of Business Research, 33 (2), 129- 142. 42 Schulz, D. P. (1996a). The nation’s biggest retail companies. Stores, (July), S2-SZ4. Schulz D. P. (1996b). New program helps independents survive against national competition. Stores (May), 44-45. Schulz, D. P. (1996c). Ace expands through beefed-up retail support. Stores (September), 83-84. Smith, G. E., Venkatraman, M. P. & Wortzel, L. H. (1995). Strategic marketing fit in manufacturer-retailer relationships: Price leaders versus merchandise differentiators. Journg ofRetaili_ng Z_1_ (3), 297-315. Smith, K. G.,. Grimm, C. M., Gannon, M. J. & Chen, M.J. (1991). Organizational information processing, competitive responses, and performance in the US. domestic airline industry. Academy of Management Journal, 35 (1), 60-85. Smith, K. G., Grimm, C. M., Wally, S. and Young, G. (1997). Strategic groups and rivalrous firm behavior: Towards a reconciliation. Strategic Management J ourn_al_, Q (2), 149-157. Stankevich, D. G. (1996). Ace of diamonds. Discount Merchandiser, fl (8), 28, 30, 32, 35, 37. Struch, N. and Schwartz, S. H. (1989). Intergroup aggression: Its predictors and distinctness from in-group bias. Joumg of Personalig and Social Psychology, & (3), 364-373. State of the Industry. (1996, August). The Chain Store Age 100. Chain Store Age, p. 3A-8A. The Industry’s Annual Report. (1996, November). Market Measure ‘97. Do-It-Yourself Retailing. Thomas, K. W. (1992). Conflict and negotiation processes in organizations. In Dunnette, MD. (Ed.), Handboplr of industrial and organizational paychology. Chicago: Rand McNally. Turner, J. H. (1991). The Structure of Sociological Theory. California: Wadsworth, Inc. U. S. Department of Commerce. (1985). 1982 Censua of Retail Trade, Industg Series, Establishment and Firm Size. (RC82-I-1). Bureau of the Census. U. S. Department of Commerce. (1990). 1987 Census of Retail Trade, Subject Series, Establishment and Firm Size. (RC87-S-1). Bureau of the Census. 43 U. S. Department of Commerce. (1995). 1992 Census of Retail Trada, Subject Series, Establishment aad Firm Size. (RC92-S-1). Bureau of the Census. Vinokur-Kaplan, D. (1995). Treatment teams that work (and those that don’t): An application of Hackrnan’s group effectiveness model to interdisciplinary teams in psychiatric hospitals. Journal of Applied Behavioral Science. 3_1(3), 303-327. Williams, J. M. and Widmeyer, W. N. (1991). The cohesion-performance outcome relationship in coacting sport. Jourpg of Sport a_n_d Exercise Psychology, Q (4), 364-371. Figures Structural Influences Characteristics of: Parties Relationship between Parties Environment Conflict Process Awareness of conflict Behaviors Outcomes ; AClapted fi'om Thomas (1992). Figure 1. General Conflict Model 44 45 Behavior Goal Conflict (htcome Figure 2. Theoretical Model 46 Figure 3. Initial Model 47 WM StrmumlMxH Warsaw X2= 160.553, df= 137, p= .083 )G= 12.414, df=8, p= .134 " p<.05 NFI=.982 NFI=.9SD ” p<.01 NNF1=.997 NNFI=.993 a fimdforithnifimtim (F1=.997 (Fl=.996 Figure 4. Revised Model Tables Table 1. Item Content: Group Identification Scale Label Statements Group Identification V5 When someone criticizes my coop, it feels like a personal insult. V6 I am very interested in what others think about my coop. V7 When I talk about my coop, I usually say we rather than they. V8 My coop’s successes aremysuccesses. V9 When someone praises my coop, it feels like a personal compliment. V10 If a story in the media criticized my coop, I would feel annoyed. 48 49 Table 2. Item Content: Goal Conflict Scale/Factor Statements Goal Conflict V12 doesn’t play by the rules that allow each of us to profitably serve out respective market niches. V13 The relationship between my store and is harmonious, allowing each of us to profitably serve our respective market niches. V14 There is mutual antagonism between my store and V15 There is implicit cooperation between my store and . V16 has a winner takes all attitude towards my store. 50 Table 3. Item Content: Behavior Factor Label Statements Quality V17 To Offer more high quality products/services V18 To emphasize more strict quality control V20 To improve employee productivity and operations efficiency V21 To provide more extensive customer service V22 To emphasize cost reduction in all facets of business operations Finance V23 To maximize use of outside financing to help gain advantage V24 To fiequently explore for new sources of funds to help gain advantage Forecasting V25 To actively attempt to predict industry trends to gain advantage V26 To actively attempt to predict competitors’ moves to gain advantage V27 To actively attempt to predict customer tastes and requirements to gain advantage Advertising V28 To use more sharply-focused advertising V29 To use more frequent advertising V30 To use advertising which clearly differentiates my products/services V31 To sponsor more frequent promotional activities Customer Service V32 To extend more generous customer credit V33 To offer a wider range of products/services V34 To Offer products/services at lower price 51 Table 4. Item Content: Outcome Scale/Factor Label Statements Performance Outcome V35 Profitability of your store (fiscal 1996) V36 Percent dollar sales growth of your store V37 Percent profit growth of your store Table 5. Sample Characteristics 52 Freq % Mean Freq '/o Mean Years 35.61 Square Footage 13,687 .37 1-10 66 20% < 5,000 70 70% 11-25 102 32% 5,000 - 10,000 148 47% 26-50 80 25% 10,001 - 15,000 38 12% 51-75 35 11% 15,001 - 25,000 27 9% 76-100 26 8% 25,001 - 50,000 20 6% > 100 13 4% > 50,000 9 3% # Stores 1.31 1996 Sales Volume $2,233,1 17 1 253 82% < $500,000 54 20% 2 36 12% $500,000 - 999,999 84 29% 3 11 4% $1,000,000 - 1,499,999 51 18% 4 3 1% $1,500,000 - 4,999,999 71 25% 5 2 1% $5,000,000 - 9,999,999 16 6% 6 3 1% > $10,000,000 9 3% 7 l 0% Full-Time Employees 11.10 1-5 141 44% 6-10 92 29% 11-15 37 12% > 15 48 15% Table 6. Representativeness of Sample 53 One-sample t-tests Variable # Cases Mean Test Value t-value df Sig— average volume 1 15 996774 913498 1.10 1 14 .275 average square footage 132 7879 7000 1.93 131 .056 average # employees 103 9.27 9 .37 102 .713 Table 7. Non-response Bias 54 t-test for equality of means Early Respondents Late Respondents Scale Mean S.D. Mean S.D. t-value sig Group Identification 23.3025 4.445 22.2000 5.531 .85 .406 Goal Conflict 12.3193 3.659 12.4500 4.322 -. 13 .899 Behavior 67.8151 11.776 68.0000 1 1.21 1 -.O7 .947 Performance Outcome 9.5304 2.583 9.7000 2.203 -.31 .759 Table 8. Measurement Model Results 55 Internal Consistency Composite Coefficient Variance Construct Reliability Alpha Extracted Group Identification .75 .80 .43 Awareness of Goa] Conflict .72 .77 .46 Quality Behavior .76 .79 .52 Control Behavior .75 .80 .51 Advertising Behavior .75 .72 .51 Performance Outcome .77 .81 .54 Table 8. (cont’d). 56 Parameter Estimates Standardized Path Parameter Standardized Residual Construct Labels Estimate t-value Estimate Variance Group Identification V5 F 1 .559 6.315" .626 .780 V6 F1 .534 6.683“ .647 .763 V9 F1 .737 9.102“I .803 .597 V10 F1 .689 8.962“ .801 .599 Goal Conflict V12 F2 .651 7.209" .713 .701 V14 F2 .726 8.517" .833 .553 V16 F2 .649 7.232" .685 .728 Quality Behavior V17 F3 .479 7.847" .700 .714 V20 F3 .602 10.042“I .868 .497 V21 F3 .464 9.406“ .820 .572 Control Behavior V23 F4 .559 5.088" .536 .844 V25 F4 .738 9.919" .866 .500 V26 F4 .789 9.986" .898 .440 Advertising Behavior V28 F6 .812 8.979“I .81 1 .585 V29 F6 .673 7.979" .754 .657 V30 F6 .782 9.604" .807 .590 Outcome V35 F9 .570 7.450“I .697 .717 V36 F9 .631 8.051 “ .743 .669 V37 F9 .798 11.546" .958 .288 "' p > .05 " p > .01 Overall Fit Chi-squareI df I p mlmlcml I Measurement Model 160.55 I 137 I .083 .982 I .997 I .997 I Table 9. Structural Model 57 Overall Fit I Chi-squareIdf pINFIINNFIICFI1 [Structural Model 12.41 I 8 .134 I .990 I .993 J .996 I Second Order Factor: Behavior Standardized Path Parameter Standardized Residual Parameter Labels Estimate t-value Estimate Variance Behavior -r Quality Behavior F8 F3 .873 7.857“ .742 .671 Behavior -r Control Behavior F8 F4 .907 8.031" .770 .638 Behavior -r Advertising Behavior F8 F6 1000' .845 .534 Path Estimates Standardized Parameter Standardized Residual Path Estimate t-value Estimate Variance Group Identification -* F1 F2 .151 1.651‘ .146 .989 Awareness of Goal Conflict Group Identification -* Behavior F1 F8 .303 3.608" .345 .934 Awareness of Goal Conflict -r F2 F8 .044 .564 .052 Behavior Behavior -' Outcome F8 F9 .006 .056 .006 1.000 parameter fixed for identification purposes "' p> .05 " p>.01 Appendices Appendix A Appendix A. Patterns and Dimensions of Rivalry Patterns of Rivalry Between Groups Within Groups Between Individual Units I Causes 1 Factors Chen 1996 Interfirrn rivalry: underlying awareness, action motivation to act, capability to act Motives MacCrimmon interpersonal & Messick rivalry: 1976 aggression, competition, proportionate competition Baum & Korn interfirm rivalry: 1996 mutual forbearance 58 59 Effects Patterns of Rivalry Between Groups Within Groups Between Individual Units Effects on performance Cool & Dierickx 1993 rivalry between strategic groups affects industry & firm profits Cool & Dierickx 1 993 rivalry within strategic groups affects industry &. firm profits Brahm 1995 excessive interfrrm rivalry results in industry ROR below competitive rate Cool, Dierickx & Jernison 1989 intensity of interfirrn rivalry influences risk- return outcomes Schul, Davis & Hartline 1995 extended interfirm rivalry, mediated by strategy, influences firm ROA & sales Hanna 1993 sibling rivalry resulted in poor athletic performance Functions of rivalry Irish1964 functions of sibling rivalry include: socialization, conflict resolution, intro to rights of others, develop role model IhuunnsofRnwdnr ‘DfiflfinChoups hfiflerar Chenl996 Funnanih Buhnnaflu' 1985 family camfldhfion vanabks: rehfiveagqqu dfifinenca finnflyshe,bhflr onhu,gmuku Chara: Ihunbnck 1995 fhnruze,finn wrench” finn nnponnvnmna toactdrunpond, Penmn& Thcnum1994 cognfihw caugofiznknrof rehfionflfips (uuuuunnytued bymsr) Peuuaf1993 rnenflxmflfip dahutrfld numflxusvusus mavenUanm 61 Patterns of Rivalry Between Groups Within Groups Between Individual Units External - Miller & market characteristics Chen 1996 characteristics: of environment market diversity, market growth, market uncertainty Baum & Korn market 1996 concentration, intensity of rivalry Cool, intensity of Dierickx & rivalry Jemison 1989 Brahm 1995 national targeting policy Schul, Davis supplier & buyer & Hartline power & threat 1995 of substitutes 62 Patterns of Rivalry Between Groups Within Groups Between Individual Units Characteristics of Relationship Mehra interdependence: 1996 resources of strategic groups, customers of strategic groups Buhrmester & quality of sibling Furman 1990 relationships: warmth, power, conflict Chen 1996 interdependence: market commonality, resource similarity Porac & interdependence: Thomas 1994 technological similarity, market similarity Baum & Kom interdependence: 1996 market domain overlap, multi- market contact Porter 1980 interdependence: market segments 63 Patterns of Rivalry Between Groups Mthin Groups Between Individual Units fl —; Characteristics Chen & speed & visibility of Hambrick Moves/Actions 1995 Chen & move Miller 1994 characteristics: visibility difliculty, centrality Chen, Smith characteristics of & Grimm actions: 1992 competitive impact, attack intensity, implementation requirement, type of action (strategic, tactical); characteristics of responses: # of responses, response I Miller & simplicity, Chen 1996 complexity Appendix B Appendix B. Questionnaire Michigan State University 1997 Study of Retail Hardware Cooperatives Section]. LAreyouamemberofaretailhardwarebuyingcooperative? yes no lfno, please do not continue with the survey, but please do return it followingthe directions on the back page. Thank you. 2. Howmanyretailhardwarebuyingcooperativesdoyoubelongto? 3. Hmwhatretailhardwarebuyingcooperativedoyoubelongto? 4. lfmeflnnmmwhididoyoudependforthegreateupaeanofymupurelnses? Pleaseamwerallquestionsinthismrveyabout‘yourcooperative’irrreferencetothecooperativeyouansweredfor#3or#4above. Section II. Toanswerthequestions inSection lelease cirelethemrrnberthatshowsyour level ofageementwiththestatement. STRONGLY NEUTRAL STRONGLY DISAGREE AGREE 1. Whensorneonecritieizesmycooperative, itfeelslikeaperaonalinsult. 1 2 3 4 5 2. Iamveryinterestcdinwhatothersthinkaboutmycooperative. l 2 3 4 5 3. Whenltalkaboutmycooperative, 1 usually say ‘we’ ratherthan ‘they'. 1 2 3 4 5 4. My cooperative’s successes aremysuccesses. 1 2 3 4 5 5. Whenaomeonepraisamywopaativeitfeeblikeapersonalcomplimem. l 2 3 4 5 6. lfastoryinthemediacriticizcdmycooperative,1wouldfeelamoyed. 1 2 3 4 5 7. lngeneraLlfavorastrmgerwhasisonthemarketingofuiedandu'ue l 2 3 4 5 productsoraervicea 8. lntlnpastSyearalhavemarketedwrymanynewlinesofwoductsor 1 2 3 4 5 aerviceainrnystore. 9. InflupastSyeargflieehangesinpmductsorservicesinmystorehave 1 2 3 4 5 beenmostlyofaminornature. 10. IngeneraLlhaveastrongtendencytochoosehigh—riskprojects(with 1 2 3 4 5 chanceaofveryhighretm'ns). ll. IngeneraLIbelieveitisbesttoexplorethehardwanreuilenvirormerl 1 2 3 4 5 yadually via timid. incremental behaviors. 12. When confronted with decision-making situations involving mmrtainty, l 1 2 3 4 5 typicallyadoptabold aggressivepostureinordertomardmizethe probability of exploiting potential opportunities. SeetlonIII. l. Inyourtown/cityorneighboringarea,isthereanotherhflwaustorewhoisamenherotaretaflhuylngeooperaflve? ._____Y“ ___no Ifno,pleasegotopage3andcornplete SectionVIIonly. Ifyes, please complete all sections ofthe survey. Thankyou. 2. lfyawhatistbenameofthestore?(Iftherehmmflnnmwhichmeposesthebiggeaflneagrealorpotanial,myomstom?) (m) 3. Whathardwareeooperatlveisthestoreinn afliliatedwith? (coowl‘lflvc) 65 SectionIV. Pleancheledummbadmwinghowimpmtunachievingudiuatanauisfwyournm. Fora‘hlank’wlthlnaqrsestiomtflnlrof thestoreyounentlonethecflonlqunesflonfl. ForenmpleifyouidanifyflappyfludwareasalocaLcooperative-afiliated store,insertflappyflariwareintheblanks. NOT AT ALL MODERATELY EXTREMELY IMPORTANT IMPORTANT IMPORTANT . To use more sharply-focused advertising than (store). 1 2 3 4 5 2. To use more frequent advertising than (store). 1 2 3 4 S 3. To use advertising which clearly difl‘erentiates my products/services from 1 2 3 4 5 those of (store). 4. To sponsor more fi'equent promotional activities (community events, how-to 1 2 3 4 5 clinics, celebrity appearances, etc.) than (store). 5. To sponsor more sharply-focused promotional activities than (store). 1 2 3 4 5 6. To sponsor promotional activities which clearly difl‘erenttate' my store fiom l 2 3 4 5 (store). To offer more big: quality products/services than (store). 1 2 3 4 5 To emphasize more strict quality control than (store). I 2 3 4 5 To provide a more attractive, customer-fiiendly physical store environment I 2 3 4 5 than (store). 10. To improve employee productivity and operations efficiency more than _ l 2 3 4 5 (store). 11. To provide more extensive customer service/support than (store). 1 2 3 4 5 l2. Toinitiatecustomerservice/mpportprognnumorefiequerulytlnn 1 2 3 4 5 (store). 13. Toactivelyatternpttopredictindustrytrendstogainadvantageover l 2 3 4 5 (“0") l4. Toadivelyattempttopredictcompetitors’movestogainadvunageover l 2 3 4 5 (store). 15. Toactivelyanernpttopredictcustomertastesmdrequiremernstogain l 2 3 4 5 advantage over (store). 16. Toenphasizeconreductioninallfacetsofbusineasoperafionsaolcannnke 1 2 3 4 5 moreprofitthan (store). 17. Tomaximizeuseofoutside financingtohelpgainadvantageover 1 2 3 4 5 (store). 18. Tofiequerulyexplorefornewsourcesoffimdstohelpgainadvantageover 1 2 3 4 5 (store). 19. To extend more generous customer credit than (store). 1 2 3 4 5 20. To offer a wider range of products/services than (store). 1 2 3 4 5 21. To offer a range of products/services which clearly differentiates my store 1 2 3 4 5 from __ (store). 22. To ofl‘er products/services at a substantially lower price than (store). 1 2 3 4 5 Section V. Pleasecirclethemunbaflutshowsyourlevelofagreunemwiflithestatanau. Forqneatlonsl-4,whereyouencountera‘blanlr’ wflhlnaqnudomthhkofthecoognflveyoumflonethecflonnanesflonfl. Forexample,ifyouidentityllappyllardware ualoeaLcoopuafive—afliliatednaemdflappyfiudwmisambaofAceCoopauiw,insertAceintheblanks. STRONGLY NEUTRAL STRONGLY DISAGREE AGREE l. lfeelastrongsemeofflrernandm”betweenmernbersofmyretail l 2 3 4 5 cooperativeandmembersof cooperative. 2. Youcanneverreallytrustmembersof cooperative. 1 2 3 4 5 1feelthatmembersofmyretailcooperativeandmembersof 1 2 3 4 5 cooperativearereallyonoppositesidesinmyindustry. 4. 1fmyretailcooperativegroupdidn’texist,membersof 1 2 3 4 5 cooperativewouldtrytodrivemembersofmycoopyoupoutofbusiness. 66 ForqnestioasS—l3,rrhereyonencomstera‘bhnk’wiflfllaquesflomtiflnkofflsestoreyonwrotehthebhnlriISectionIII, question #2. STRONGLY NEUTRAL STRONGLY DISAGREE AGREE 5. There is unspoken agreement between my store and (store) 1 2 3 4 5 aboutourrespectivemarketnichesintheretailhardwareindustry. 6. (store) doesn‘t ‘play by the rules’ that allow each of us to 1 2 3 4 5 profitably serve our respective market niches. 7. The relationship between my store and (store) is harmonious, 1 2 3 4 5 allowing each of us to profitably serve our respective market niches. . Thereismuuralantagonismbetweenmystoreand (store). 1 2 3 4 5 9. There is implicit cooperation between my store and (store). 1 2 3 4 5 10. (store)hasa‘wirmertakes all‘attitudetowardsmy 1 2 3 4 5 store. 11. Inrelationto (store),ltypicallyrespondtoactionswhich 1 2 3 4 5 12. Inrelationto (store),lamveryofienthefirsttointroduce l 2 3 4 5 new products/services, operating technologies, etc. 13. In relation to (store), I typically seek to avoid competitive 1 2 3 4 S clashes, preferring a ‘live-and-let-live‘ posture. Section VI. In comparison to other retail hardware cooperatives, please show your store’s and your cooperative’s performance on these dirnenaions: MUCH BELOW SAME AS MUCH ABOVE INDUSTRY INDUSTRY INDUSTRY AVERAGE AVERAGE AVERAGE 1. Profitability of your store (fiscal 1996) 1 2 3 4 5 2. Percent Dollar Sales Growth of your store (fiscal 1996) 1 2 3 4 5 3. Percent ProfitGrowthofyourstore(fiscal 1996) l 2 3 4 S 4. Profitability of your cooperative group (fiscal 1996) 1 2 3 4 5 5. Percent Dollar Sales Growth of your cooperative group (fiscal 1996) 1 2 3 4 5 6. Percent Profit Growth of your cooperative group (fiscal 1996) l 2 3 4 5 Section VII. Please answer the following questions about characteristics of your store. 1. Moststoreshaveseveral competitors intheirtradingarea. Ofthecornpetitorsinyourtndingareawhatonestore isyourlnost hportant competitor? 2. Whattypeofstore isthiscompetitor(circleone)? Big Box Cooperative Independent National Chain Regional Chain Wholesale Voluntary Other Howmanyyearshasyouroperationbeeninbusiness? years Howmanystoreswerethereinyouroperationinfiscal 1996? Howmanypeople, on average, did you employ in your operation in fiscal 1996? full-time part-time Whatistheapproximatesquarefootageofyourtotaloperation? sq.fi. Whatsales growth/decline didyourtotal operation achieve infiscal 1996? % growth, or %decline qudieapprordmatesdesvolumeofyourtetalopaationinfiscallm? S ”$9995" 67 Thankyonforparticipathginthisreaeareh. Toreturnthequestionrrair'e.foldthebooklet intothirdsalongthehorizontallineswiththeremmaddressandpostageontheoutside,tapeor stapleitclosed,andputitinthemail. Toensureconfidemialityfionotpdyourmreturnaddressonmebooldet. Michigan State University Department of Human Environment and Design Merchandising Management 204 Human Ecology Building East Lansing, MI 48824-1030 LESLIE STOEL DEPARTMENT OF HUMAN ENVIRONMENT AND DESIGN MERCHANDISING MANAGEMENT 204 HUMAN ECOIDGY BUILDING EAST LANSING, MI 48824-1030 Appendix C Appendix C. Formulas for Composite Reliability and Variance Extracted Construct Reliability = LSum of Wings)’ (Sum of standardized loadings)2 + sum of indicator measurement error Variance Extracted = Sum of squared standardized loadings Sum of squared standardized loadings + sum of indicator measurement error From: Hair, J.F., Jr., Anderson, R.E., Tatham, KL. and Black, WC. (1995). Multivariate Data sis with Readings, Fourth Edition. Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey: Prentice Hall, p. 653. 68