Lung--. «0.9. . L. I 0‘ . .4 sfimfifi . v : $.11th . I .u .. : Isl! 1.. I. l ... 1 WWW.) . .- Immuwfl». turn “I. '1‘.-. .. 5.55... -\.| 35.535.595.525. . .l..\ 55.55 THESIS ll llllllllllll LIBRARY Michigan State University This is to certify that the thesis entitled THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN SOCIOECONOMIC STATUS AND PREVALENCE OF BEHAVIORAL RISK FACTORS FOR STROKE AMONG OLDER ADULTS presented by Trudy Ellen Day has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for ._M._S_.__degree in M. n Major professor Date /2//0/?8 MS U is an Affirmative Action/Equal Opportunity Institution PLACE IN REI'URN Box to remove this checkout from your record. To AVOID FINIS return on or before date due. MAY BE RECALLED with earlier due date if requested. DATE DUE DATE DUE DATE DUE tttttt W W14 THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN SOCIOECONOMIC STATUS AND PREVALENCE OF BEHAVIORAL RISK FACTORS FOR STROKE AMONG OLDER ADULTS By Trudy Ellen Day A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fiilfillment of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF SCIENCE College of Nursing 1998 ABSTRACT THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN SOCIOECONOMIC STATUS AND PREVALENCE OF BEHAVIORAL RISK FACTORS FOR STROKE AMONG OLDER ADULTS By Trudy Ellen Day Recognition of socioeconomic status (SES) as a predisposing factor for engaging in unhealthy lifestyle behaviors could potentially yield major dividends in stroke prevention Directing interventions that change high risk lifestyle behaviors at those persons at greatest risk would make the most emcient and efl‘ective use of scarce health care resources. This study’s purpose was to determine the relationship between SES and prevalence of behavioral risk factors for stroke among older adults. Williams’ (1990) theory on SES and health outcomes provided the framework for this study. This non- experimental descriptive study was conducted using data from the Northern Michigan Community Health Assessment (1995) survey which contained questions on income, education, and behavioral risk factors—hypertension (H'I'N), smoking, alcohol consumption, blood cholesterol, obesity, and physical inactivity. Subjects aged 55 and older were included in this secondary data analysis; a stratified simple random sample totaling 2,710 older adults. Prevalence of behavioral risk factors by income and education was determined using frequency counts which show a higher prevalence of HTN, current smoker, obesity, and inactivity among participants in the lowest income and education level. Chi-Square test revealed that there were significant differences in prevalence of some behavioral risk factors for income and education. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS This thesis would not be possible without the invaluable assistance of my committee chairperson, Sharon King. I am grateful that she graciously accepted the position of chairperson and aided me in identifying additional committee members. I would like to express my sincere appreciation for her expertise in thesis writing and research, her proficiency with electronic technology, and her swifi feedback after reviewing drafts of this thesis. The positive feedback I received fi'om Sharon was really what encouraged me to continue though the research process and made me truly proud of my work. I wish to extend my thankfulness to Patty Peck and Laura Struble, committee members, for the time they devoted to reading my thesis proposal and final drafi and their suggestions for change. Their recommendations served to elevate the quality of my work. I am equally indebted to Robert Sloan and Minda Latham for their knowledge of and assistance with the research process; and more importantly for enabling me to access the survey data. I do appreciate the time expended answering all my questions on the methods used to conduct the primary research study. Lastly, many, many thanks to Gary and Molly who stood by me during this work and supported me emotionally. I am fortunate to have such loyal supporters. Please share in my delight at the completion of this huge undertaking. iii TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES .......................................................................................................... vi LIST OF FIGURES ....................................................................................................... vii INTRODUCTION ........................................................................................................... 1 PROBLEM STATEMENT .............................................................................................. 3 THEORETICAL FRAMEWORK .................................................................................... 4 REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE .................................................................................. 8 Conceptual Definitions of the Variables .............................................................. 10 Socioeconomic Status (8138) .................................................................. 10 Behavioral Risk Factors (BRF) ............................................................... 12 SES and Single Behavioral Risk Factors ............................................................. 15 SES and Multiple Behavioral Risk Factors ......................................................... 19 METHODS ................................................................................................................... 21 Sample .............................................................................................................. 21 Field Procedures ................................................................................................ 22 Data Collection Procedures and Recording ......................................................... 22 Prctection of Human Rights ............................................................................... 24 Operational Definitions of the Variables ............................................................. 24 SES ........................................................................................................ 24 Behavioral Risk Factors (BRF) ............................................................... 25 Limitations ......................................................................................................... 26 Instrumentation .................................................................................................. 28 Scoring and Data Summarizing Procedures ........................................................ 29 Research Design ................................................................................................ 30 Data Processing and Statistical Analysis ............................................................. 30 RESULTS ..................................................................................................................... 31 Demographics .................................................................................................... 31 Prevalence of Behavioral Risk Factors ................................................................ 33 Significance in Difference of Proportions ............................................................ 37 Interpretation of Findings ................................................................................... 39 DISCUSSION ............................................................................................................... 42 Limitations ......................................................................................................... 42 Implications for Advanced Nursing Practice and Primary Care ............................ 43 Recommendations for Further Research .............................................................. 48 Summary ............................................................................................................ 49 iv APPENDIX A - UCRIHS Approval Letter .................................................................... 51 BIBLIOGRAPHY ......................................................................................................... 54 LIST OF TABLES Table 1 - Distribution of Study Population by Income and Education .............................. 32 Table 2 - Prevalence of Individual Behavioral Risk Factors by Income and Education ................................................................................. 34 Table 3 - Prevalence of Total Number of Behavioral Risk Factors by Income and Education ................................................................................. 35 Table 4 - Chi-Square for Individual and Total Behavioral Risk Factors by Income and Education ................................................................................ 38 LIST OF FIGURES Figure l - A Paradigm for Research on Socioeconomic Status and Health Williams, 1990, p.81) ..................................................................................... 5 Figure 2 - Schematic Representation of Williams’ (1990) Paradigm With Study Variables ............................................................................................... 9 Figure 3 - Schematic Representation of Williams’ (1990) Paradigm With Study Variables, Revised ............................................................................... 41 INTRODUCTION Stroke is major cause of disability and the third leading cause of death in the United States and Michigan (U .8. Preventive Services Task Force, 1996; Michigan Department of Public Health, 1993). It is a major public health problem and the most common life threatening neurologic disease that affects older adults. With an estimated prevalence of 3 million stroke survivors, this disease places enormous burdens on family members and caretakers, often necessitating skilled care in an institutional setting. The direct and indirect costs of stroke in the United States have been estimated at $30 billion annually (U .8. Preventive Services Task Force, 1996). Many of the leading causes of death are associated with unhealthy lifestyles and high risk behaviors. For this reason, Healthy People 2000 (U .S. Department of Health and Human Services, 1990) established a national strategy to improve the health of all Americans. Its purpose is to promote health and prevent disease through changes in lifestyle and enviromnental factors. To accomplish this, broad public health goals with specific objectives and separate priority areas that provide direction for the 10-year-drive were established. Stroke was identified as one of several priority areas to target since many stroke risk factors are related to lifestyle, and therefore are modifiable. Healthy Michigan 2000 (Michigan Department of Public Health, 1993) also included stroke as an area to target with a goal to reduce the number of stroke deaths. Essential to reducing the incidence and prevalence of stroke is the identification of causative factors. Risk factors for stroke in relation to unhealthy lifestyle and behaviors are hypertension, smoking, alcohol intake, elevated blood cholesterol, obesity, and physical inactivity (Kelly-Hayes, 1991; Haheirrr, Holrne, Hjermann & Leren, 1993). Considerable evidence has accumulated demonstrating that maximizing healthy behaviors and minimizing risky lifestyles can reduce the incidence of stroke (Dunbabin & Sandercock, 1990; Wolf, 1990; Matcher, McCrory, Barnett & Feussner, 1994). It has been postulated that the decline in stroke mortality in the early 19703 was attributable to increased control of hypertension by use of antihypertensive medications. In addition, other lifestyle factors probably influenced the decline both directly and indirectly, for example, reduction in use of cigarettes and reduction in sodium and alcohol intake on blood pressure respectively (Casper, ng, Strogatz, Davis & Tyroler, 1992; Jacobs, Mcgovem & Blackburn, 1992). This supports that risk factors can be eliminated or modified which is the basis for primary and secondary prevention (Wolf, D’Agostino, Belanger & Karmel, 1991). Equally important for reducing the incidence of stroke is the identification of predisposing factors for engaging in unhealthy lifestyles and high risk behaviors (W ister, 1996). AS noted in Healthy People 2000, “Health disparities between poor people and those with higher incomes are almost universal for all dimensions of health.” (U. S. Department of Health and Human Services, 1990, p. 29) Socioeconomic status (SES) has been described as one of the strongest and most consistent predictors of a person’s mortality and morbidity (kaleby, Jatulis, Frank & F ortmann, 1992). Mster (1996) found that SES affects health behaviors in relatively important ways but this depends on the measure, specific behavior and the age group. Most studies examine associations between a Single measure of SES and a single disease risk factor, or compare the mortality and morbidity experience of different SES groups. (Jefl‘ery & French, 1996; Matthews, Kelsey, Meilahn, Kuller & ng, 1989;Jacobsen & Thelle, 1988). Recognition of SES— specifically, income and education as a predisposing factor for high risk behaviors could potentially yield major dividends in disease prevention. A reduction in risk factors at any age can have a positive impact on health outcomes. More research on identifying persons prone to engaging in unhealthy behaviors, could aid health care practitioners in impacting stroke incidence. Determining the extent to which SES is related to high risk behavior could assist with the identification of interventions that change lifestyle behavior and enhance the health of older adults. Understanding the association between measures for SES and a set of risk factors for stroke, risk factor reduction can be launched by essential plamring, services, and education Educational and behavioral interventions, provided in the primary care setting and directed at those persons at greatest risk, would make the most eficient and efl‘ective use of scarce health care resources. PROBLEM STATEMENT Examining the association between income, education and a set of risk factors for stroke-namely, hypertension, smoking, excessive alcohol consumption, elevated blood cholesterol, obesity, and physical inactivity could conceivably aid in identifying the stroke- prone person. Study of the prevalence of these risk factors in different socioeconomic groups among older adults may lead to the possibility of focusing preventive measures on areas where maximum effect may be gained. Therefore, the purpose of this study is to answer the question: “Is there a sigmficant relationship between SES and prevalence of behavioral risk factors for stroke mnong older adults in Northern Michigan?” Two hypotheses will be tested in this study. The hypotheses are: (1) higher prevalence of behavioral risk factors-hypertension, smoking, excessive alcohol consumption, elevated blood cholesterol, obesity, and physical inactivity is associated with lower income; and (2) higher prevalence of behavioral risk factors—hypertension, smoking, excessive alcohol consumption, elevated blood cholesterol, obesity, and physical inactivity is associated with lower educational attainment. THEORETICAL FRAMEWORK The fiamework for this study will be explained using erliams’ (1990) theory on SES and health. Williams (1990) provides an explanatory framework for predicting health outcomes. The relationship of the study variables to the fi'amework will be described. Williams (1990) posits SES as an important determinant of health status in his Paradigm for Research on Socioeconomic Status and Health. According to Williams (1990), social status shapes individual values and behaviors, consequently linking social structure and health outcomes. This perspective predicts that because “social structures Shape individual values and behaviors, SES differentials in morbidity and mortality are due at least in part to conditions in life that derive from an individual’s structural position” (p. 81). In this paradigm, as illustrated in Figure 1, several factors are linked to health outcomes. Psychosocial factors refers to lifestyle characteristics and living conditions that are viewed not as individual characteristics but as the patterned response (response to stress, a way of coping) of social groups to the realities and constraints of the external environment. Such factors include: (1) health practices, as in behaviors like nutrition and smoking; (2) social ties, referring to social integration and support; (3) perceptions of control, including mastery; and (4) stress in family, occupational and or residential environment. This paradigm suggests that variations in the type and quantity of stress at .22 .83 52.535 5.5. a... 2:5 35:88.98 .3 £885. 5 55:... < ._ 2.5...— .82 can .82 .92 25.3 85 .8. E38."— can—mason 5 u _ _ \r \r\ \\ _ _ _ 5 5 _ _ /1 I. _ _ _ . _ _ _ _ _ 53:858. gauges .58: Spam 3. .258 .3 S38? 3 a: 38m a 38.53. 4283 age 8898 £8: a 238m 388%?— work or in the home are linked to socioeconomic position. Correspondingly, smoking, alcohol use, health enhancing attitudes, and social ties depends on the social and economic structures and arrangements of society (Williams, 1990). High levels of social support may minimize the need to use unhealthy coping strategies, such as smoking and alcohol use, to modify stress (V enters, 1989). Another factor linked to health outcomes in this paradigm is SES. Williams (1990) postulates that SES is predictive of health outcomes. An adequate theoretical understanding of the relationship between social stratification and health status must include the role played by psychosocial factors. These factors are concomitants of social status and they represent the pathways through which the effects of social stratification are mediated to individuals. Nelson (1994) points out that social stratification results in the concentration of the poor in low-status, physically demanding, and less rewarding jobs in which they also experience greater risks of unemployment, exposure to occupational hazards, and increased psychological stress. Medical care, another factor within this paradigm, alludes to access to and utilization of health care. Psychosocial factors and medical care are viewed as linked to social status and can be mediators of the association between SES and health outcomes. Both of these factors are posited to affect health outcomes either by direct additive relationships as illustrated in Figure 1 (indicated by a thin, solid line), or by interactive relationships with social status (indicated by a dotted line). In other words, lower SES persons not only may receive more exposure to psychosocial risk factors and deficits in medical care but also may be more vulnerable to them (Kessler, 1979; Williams, 1990). The relationship between these two explanatory factors-medical care and psychosocial factors may also be reciprocal. Medical care, especially preventative care, can influence psychosocial factors directly (indicated by a thin, solid line); and psychosocial factors can afl‘ect patterns of health care utilization (Williams, 1990). Some researchers argue that social factors are even more important determinants of health status than the availability of medical care and health knowledge. Disease states are more clearly related to social phenomena such as stress and industrialization (Cassel, 1976). Each additional dollar spent on education, formal or health education, improves health status more than each additional dollar Spent on medical care. Health education achieves only limited success however, and is more effective in producing behavior change in higher SES persons (Slater & Carlton, 1985). It has been noted that more educated persons are more aware of health risks and more likely to initiate interventions to reduce these risks (Cockerharn, Lueschen, Kunz & Spaeth, 1986). Adopting health enhancing behavior is not simply the result of greater health knowledge. Formal education has pervasive efi‘ects on health apart from health knowledge. Persons with less formal education face different stnrctural constraints, have more stress and fewer resources to cope with this stress. Risk factors for distant health outcomes may be the basic survival strategies of day-to-day existence for low SES persons (Venters, 1989). Additional factors linked to health outcomes within this paradigm are demographic factors and biomedical factors. Demographic factor includes age, race and sex. This factor has a direct additive relationship to all other factors within the paradigm. The strength of the relationship; however, is less (indicated by a thin, solid line) than those factors linked with a thick, solid line. Biomedical factors refers to genetics or make-up of the body, and a direct additive relationship exists with psychosocial factors, health outcomes, and SES. This relationship is weak as indicated by a thin, solid line (Williams, 1990). Williams’ (1990) organizational framework provides an opportunity for examining the relationship between the study variables. Figure 2 illustrates the variables in this study (depicted in bold, lowercase letters) and their connections to health outcomes within this paradigm. The intent of this research is to focus on the relationship between SES and the prevalence of six behavioral risk factors for stroke among older adults. These variables are identified in the fiamework under socioeconomic status and psychosocial factors. The predictive study variables include income and education, and are identified under SES. The outcome variables hypertension, smoking, excessive alcohol consumption, elevated blood cholesterol, obesity, and physical inactivity are identified under health practices within psychosocial factors. Stroke risk is the health outcome of interest. In summary, this paradigm accepts explicitly that health status is the result of complex causes; it includes controls for sociodemographic factors, such as age and sex, and for early environmental, genetic, and biomedical factors. Moreover, it suggests that psychosocial factors such as health behavior, stress, and the resources to cope with stress, can account for a substantial part of the association between social status and health outcomes. lastly, this paradigrn infers that primary prevention be directed toward changes in social and economic environment rather than a person’s behavior (Williams, 1990). REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE The purpose of this Study is to determine the extent to which SES is related to the prevalence of behavioral risk factors for stroke. The literature will be reviewed to ascertain what is already known about the study variables. First, research focusing on £23.; Sam 5..: 3.5.5.. 8.3: .255...» a 8:38.23. 8.523 .N 2.5.... «E.— oxehn . ”3803.5 .2855 f $2.8... 3...... - $8.... - 358835 ecu-30.0 .. newt—8:239 =58... .. memo—ea: - noun—6.3%.— - 8388a .23: 2 £38..— Emogonozmm 55:63.0 158.85 - 2:35 .. 538m 0588080505 conceptual definitions and measurement of SES and behavioral risk factors as they relate to stroke risk will be discussed. Second, research concentrating on SES and single behavioral risk factors, as well as SES and multiple behavioral risk factors, will be examined. C l D [i . . E l I! . l l W. Whenused in this study, education and income are the independent variables used as the primary measure of SES. Education is the highest grade or year of school completed (kaleby, Fortman & Barrett, 1990). Income is annual income from all sources before taxes (Winkleby et al., 1990). A variety of approaches to the conceptualization and measurements of SES have been taken in research (Feinstein, 1993, Kaplan & Keil, 1993). Social class, social status, occupation, employment status, income, and education are some examples. Income and education are more objective measures and are therefore typically used more often (Nelson, 1994). Education is the most widely used measure of SES in epiderniologic studies. Kaplan and Keil (1993) examined SES measures in an extensive literature review and concluded that of the studies of chronic diseases published in the Americanlmmalgf Epidemiology in 1982 and 1985 in which SES was used, education was used 45% of the time as a measure of SES. The thought is that this is primarily because questions about education in surveys have relatively low nonresponse rates due to the simplicity of the question to answer. Another thought is education usually remains constant throughout adulthood and as a result is unlikely influenced by poor health among adults, unlike income and occupation (F einstein, 1993). Although education is very useful as a measure, it is not without some limitations. 10 For example, there are large cohort difl‘erences in level of education, so that the behavioral and social correlates of a given level of education may vary depending on a person’s age (Liberatos, Link & Kelsey, 1988). The value of education as a measure of SES may also differ for certain subgroups such as Afiican-Americans. For African-Americans and women, the link between education and income is weaker than it is for Caucasian men. These sub-groups receive less economic return from their education than do Caucasian men (Kaplan & Keil, 1993). The usefulness of education as a predictive variable in certain populations may be lessened due to an increasing homogeneity in educational level. Additionally, education doesn’t reflect acquired experience or continuing education—for example, certification programs, conferences and seminars. The percent of U. S. population with at least a high school education was approximately 75% in 1986, reflecting a decreased stratification of the population based on educational level (Nelson, 1994). Lastly, although illness in adulthood cannot influence level of education completed before adulthood, it is not implausible that poor health as a child could influence the amount or quality of education ultimately obtained (kaleby, et al., 1989). Measures of income are obviously an important measure of SES. Income provides access to services and goods including medical care and quality education that may protect against disease. However, lower income may reflect the influence of poorer health (Nelson, 1994). In addition, the measurement of income level is complicated. Family or individual income can be measured; income can be compared to poverty levels; family income can be adjusted for family size; sources of income other than wages can be included, such as food stamps or Medicare; and income levels may vary over time. It may also be important to measure aflluence which includes total assets. Income was reported in 11 only 15% of the research articles published in the WWW in the 1980s that had measures of SES. This could be due to the perceived sensitivity of survey respondents on income related questions. Nonresponse rates for income related questions on surveys averages 9% to 10%. Respondents need to be reassured about the confidentiality of the data they provide to reduce this mimber (Kaplan & Keil, 1993). mm For this study, BRF are the dependent variables and refer to self-reported: (1) high blood pressure; (2) smoker or heavy smoker, (3) heavy drinker or binge drinker, (4) elevated blood cholesterol; (5) overweight; and (6) inactivity (Community Health Assessment, 1995; Stein, Lederman & Shea, 1993). 1. High blood pressure, as defined by Uphold and Graham (1994), is a systolic blood pressure greater than 140 mm mercury or a diastolic blood pressure greater than 90 mm mercury. Hypertension is the most potent and common precursor of stroke. It has long been recognized as an important risk factor for stroke. The American Heart Association (1988) says that the risk of stroke rises directly with a person’s blood pressure and both systolic and diastolic pressures are important. People with hypertension sufi‘er 2 to 4 times as many strokes as those with normal blood pressure. The Behavioral Risk Factor Surveillance System (BRFS S), an ongoing surveillance program developed and funded by the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, is designed to estimate the prevalence of risk factors for the major causes of death in the US. Data from the BRFSS are a central component of federal and state activities designed to monitor progress toward achieving the Healthy 2000 health objectives (Stein, Lederman & Shea, 1993). The Behavioral Risk Factor Survey (BRFS) operationally defines hypertension as “self-reported hypertension measured as ever being told by a 12 doctor or other health professional that one has high blood pressure; or self-reported medical prescription for hypertension” (Jackson, Jatulis & Fortrnan, 1992, p.414). Another definition is, on more than one occasion blood pressure was high or is taking blood pressure medication (Stein, ct al., 1993). 2. Smoking promotes atherosclerosis and is the leading risk factor for stroke (U .8. Preventive Services Task Force, 1996). Smoking is probably the most studied lifestyle behavior afl‘ecting health. The BRFS defines smoking as self-reported smoking status—- current cigarette use, cigarette consumption and former cigarette use (Stein et al., 1993; Jackson et al., 1992). In the Nurses’Health Study of approximately 120,000 women who were followed prospectively for 8 years for the development of diseases, including stroke, current smoking was strongly associated with stroke. The relative risk increased directly with the number of cigarettes smoked. Heavy smokers (defined as more than 40 cigarettes per day) had nearly fourfold increased risk of stroke when compared with nonsmokers (Colditz, Bonita & Stampfer, 1988). In a study of male cigarette smokers, Kleg and Whelton (1987) found that up to 16,000 strokes per year in men could be attributed solely to smoking. According to the National Center for Health Statistics data ( 1990), the age- adjusted prevalence for current cigarette smoking is greatest for persons with less than 12 years of education, nearly 2 V2 times the rate for those with 16 or more years of education. 3. Heavy drinking is defined by 60 or more drinks consumed, on an average, in a month (Stein et al., 1993; Community Health Assessment, 1995). Heavy alcohol intake has been associated with strokes (Palomaki & Kaste, 1993). There is support for an increased incidence of stroke with an increase in alcohol consumption in men. Nondrinkers and heavy drinkers have a higher incidence of stroke, whereas moderate 13 users generally have a lower incidence (W annarnethee & Shaper, 1996; Hillbom & Kaste, 1982). Binge drinking is defined as five or more drinks consumed on an occasion at least once in the past month (Stein, et al., 1993; Community Health Assessment, 1995). 4. Elevated blood cholesterol is defined in the BRFS as cholesterol level 200mg/dL or higher; derived from non-fasting venous samples measured in milligrams per deciliter (Stein et al., 1993). A strong positive relationship exists between blood cholesterol levels and risk of coronary heart disease; however, less reliable evidence exists as a direct relationship between stroke incidence and cholesterol levels (Iso, Jacobs & Wentworth, 1989). It can be rationalized, however, that lowering blood cholesterol levels through diet modification and or drugs is an efi‘ective treatment for stroke prevention since those people with coronary disease are at risk for stroke (Dyken, Wolf& Barnett, 1984). 5. Obesity measured by body mass index (BMI) which is defined as wt(kg)/ht in meters squared. The mean BMI is calculated fi'om self-reported weight and height. Obese is a BMI exceeding 27.8 kg/rn2 for men and 27.3 kg/m2 for women (Winkleby et al., 1990; Stein et al., 1993; Community Health Assessment, 1995). Whether obesity contributes independently to stroke is uncertain. Obesity does, however, aggravate hypertension, heart disease and diabetes; all definite risk factors for stroke (I-Iaheim, Holrne, Hjermann & Leren, 1993). 6. Inactivity is termed sedentary lifestyle and is defined as no exercise in past month in the BRFS (Stein et al., 1993). Physical activity in the literature has been studied using leisure time physical activity, job related activity, house hold physical activity, and walking (Ford, et al., 1991). Finally, in stroke, the importance of each specific risk factor depends on two 14 factors. First, whether the risk factor itself is a high-level risk that, in itself, places a person in a higher probability range for stroke by the strength of its association with stroke. Second, the number and combinations of risk factors present that join to make a high-risk status for stroke for the person (Kelly-Hayes, 1991 ). There is a considerable body of evidence for an association between SES and mortality. The results showed an inverse relationship between level of education and mortality (Salonen, 1982; Siegel, et al., 1987; Keil, Sutherland, Knapp & Tyroler, 1992). Specifically, the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey revealed that men aged45 to64with0to 7years ofeducationhad 1.96 timestheriskofdeaththanthose with 12 or more years of education. Those with 8 to 11 years of education had 1.6 times the risk (Feldman, Makuc, Kleimnan & Coroni-Huntley, 1989). The results of the National Longitudinal Mortality Study (Roget, Sorlie, Johnson & Scmitt, 1992) indicated a strong inverse relationship between total family income and death. Caucasen males with incomes ofless than $5,000 had amortality ratio 1.8 times greaterthanthat ofmenwith incomes of $50,000 or more, and for women the ratio was 1.3. Even greater differences were seen for subgroups like Afiican-Americans. The literature has primarily addressed the influence of SES indicators on individual behavioral risk factors, but some studies have looked at multiple risk factors. Probably the most evidence for an inverse relationship between SES and risk factors has to do with hypertension (Feinstein, 1993; Kaplan & Keil, 1993). The research supports that low SES is related to both the prevalence and incidence of hypertension (Keil, Tyroler, Sandifer, & Boyle, 1977; Keil, Sandifer, Loadholt & Boyle, 1981). A statistically significant inverse 15 relation between educational level and blood pressure was found among all Caucasians subjects in the Chicago Heart Association Detection Project. This was also true for younger Afiican-American men, but for older subjects and women the relationship was not significant due to the small number of subjects in these groups (Dryer, Starnler, Shekelle & Schoenberger, 1976). A strong inverse relationship between level of current smoking and education is found in the literature (Pierce , Fiore, Novotny, Hatziandreu & Davis, 1989). Age- adjusted smoking prevalence was strongly and inversely related to educational levels for both males and females in a study conducted in the Twin Cities area. Study participants with less than a high school education were more likely to be current smokers than were other education groups. The same pattern was also true for income; low income was associated with the high prevalence of smoking in men and women (Iribarren, et al., 1997). In a study that examined the effects of SES on patterns of exercise and smoking for three age groups, Wister (1996) found that education and income exhibited statistically significant relationships with smoking. Results depended upon the age group and the specific behavior. Persons with post-secondary education were less likely to smoke than those with lower education for the two age groups under age 65. Persons who completed high school were more likely to smoke than persons with lower education for the 25 to 44 age group. However, this did not hold true for the 45 to 64, and 65 and over age groups. In contrast, income appeared to predict smoking behavior in a more uniform pattern across age groups. The middle income group was less likely to smoke than the lower income group for all age groups. The high income group was less likely to smoke than the lower income group, but only among persons aged 25 to 44 and those aged 45 to 64. No 16 relationship was supported for persons aged 65 and over. Mirand and Welte (1996) sought to estimate prevalence and observe whether active or health oriented lifestyles were related to heavy alcohol intake among the elderly. They found that the prevalence of heavy drinking among the study participants was 6%. Adjusted analysis showed positive associations between heavy drinking and being male. Negative relationships were observed between heavy drinking and SES. There are few published papers that specifically focus on the relationship between total cholesterol and SES; most focus on high density lipoproteins (the “good guys”). A study of 561 men and women, that had a 9 year follow-up, found that educational achievement was positively related to high-density lipoprotein levels for both genders (Donahue, Orchard, Kuller & Drash, 1985). Reports of a study among a population of 59,566 female volunteers in the “Take Off Pounds Sensibly” program in the US. and Canada conclude that there was an inverse relation between obesity and SES (Oken, Hartz, Giefer & Rirnrn, 1977). Another study, based on a longitudinal survey of a national probability sample of 10,039 subjects 16 to 24 years of age explored the relationship between SES and obesity. The researchers found that during the course of 17 years, overweight women married less often, had lower incomes and more of them had incomes below the poverty level, and completed fewer years of school. Similar, but weaker, trends were found among men (Gortmaker, Must, Penin, Sobel & Dietz, 1993). Levels of physical activity have consistently been shown to vary by SES, although not always in the same direction for leisure and on the job physical activity. Matthews, Kelsey, Meilalm, Kuller & ng (1989) found that middle-aged women who had an 17 advanced degree expended 56% more energy per week in nonwork related physical activities than women with a high school education or less. Similarly, those in the lowest social class had an increased risk ofbeing inactive 4 1/2 times that ofsubjects in the highest classes (Helrnert, Herman, Joeckel, Greiser & Madans, 1989). Mster (1996) examined the efl‘ects of SES on patterns of exercise and smoking for three age groups: persons age 25 to 44, 45 to 64, and 65 and over. Results indicated that among the 25 to 44 age group, persons having completed high school are 1.51 times as likely to be active compared with the lower education group (some high school or less). Statistically significant but slightly weaker associations between education and activity were found for the other two age groups. Conversely, income was not found to be an important predictor of activity level for any of the age groups. Two studies using SES measure that combined education and income, had conflicting findings. A direct relationship between SES and level of both occupational and leisure time physical activity was discovered with those in the lower class being less active (Holrne, Helgeland, Hjermann, Leren & Lund-Larsen, 1981). In contrast, lower SES men in Finland had higher levels of occupational physical activity than higher SES men (Salonen, Slater, Tuomilehto & Raurarnaa, 1988). Ford et a1. (1991), using 574 subjects living in Pittsburgh, demonstrated in his study that higher SES status women spent significantly more time each week in leisure time physical activity, job related activity, and household physical activity than did lower SES women. Lower SES men spent significantly more time each week walking and doing household chores, whereas higher SES men tended to be more active in leisure time physical activity. These data suggest important difi‘erences in activity among population subgroups. 18 SES llll'lEl . ”3.”,- There is a substantial amount of literature on the relationship between SES and individual risk factors; whereas, very little literature is available on SES and multiple risk factors. In a study examining associations between education and six behavioral risk factors for disease, a highly significant relationship was found. Those participants with lower education were at higher risk for disease. Increased education was related to lower smoking, obesity, cholesterol levels, and hypertension prevalence. The findings were remarkably significant (p < .01) for both sexes and in the young as well as older age group, with the exception of cholesterol among males and the 50 to 74 year old age group (Winkleby, et al., 1990). In a study that examined correlations among education, income, and occupation and a set of risk factors—smoking, cholesterol, blood pressure, positive correlations were found among the three SES indicators. Correlations ranged fiom 0.23 to 0.67 with the strongest being between education and occupation and the weakest being between education and income. All correlations among the three indicators and risk factors were found in an univariate analysis; when a forward regression analysis was used, only education was found to be significantly and inversely related with the risk factors (Winkleby, Jatulis, Frank & Fortmann, 1992). The association between educational attainment and behavioral risk factors for disease was studied among 541 middle-aged women in Pennsylvania. The study showed that the less educated women had higher blood pressure and higher low density lipoproteins (all p values < .01). The less educated women reported health behaviors distinctly different from those more educated. The less education the women had, the 19 more often they reported being current or ever smokers of cigarettes and expending kw kilocalories per week in physical exercise; and the less often they reported consmning alcohol one day a week or more (all p values < .01). Interestingly, there were minimal difi‘erences in diet among the women according to education (Matthews, Kelsey, Meilahn, Kuller & Wing, 1989). Cockerrnan, Lueschen, Kunz and Spaeth (1986) hypothesized that as SES increases the more likely a person will be involved in forms of health-advancing behavior; the poor will be less engaged in these behaviors than those of higher status. Income, education, and occupation were the components used for measuring SES. Food habits, participating in sports and activities, smoking status, and alcohol consumption were indicators used to reflect health-advancing behavior. The researchers found that, in 401 adult residents of Illinois, income and occupation were significant with respect to food habits; and education was significant with respect to sports and activities. Precisely, lower income persons were more likely to select their food carefully, as were persons in higher status occupations (but not necessarily with higher incomes). Thus, findings for groups opposite ends of the social spectrum appear to coincide. Persons with higher education and younger persons were significantly more likely to participate in sports and exercise than those with limited education and older. For smoking status and alcohol consumption, only sex was statistically significant in that men reported more drinking and smoking than women. Overall, the results did not lend strong support to their hypothesis. Only education was significant with respect to sports and exercise, unlike the other indicators for SES and health-advancing behaviors. In summary, there is an abundance of literature linking SES with behavioral risk 20 factors. In the few studies of multiple risk factors there has generally been an inverse relationship between SES measures and hypertension, smoking, excessive alcohol intake, blood cholesterol, and obesity. There is also evidence for an inverse relationship between individual risk factors and SES. The literature is most abundant for the relations between SES and hypertension or SES and smoking. The literature is more limited for SES and obesity or SES and physical activities; and quite scarce for examining multiple measures for SES and a set of behavioral risk factors in the older adult population The evidence for a relation between cholesterol level and SES is inconsistent. METHODS Sample Data for this study was derived from the 1995 Northern Michigan Community Health Assessment (NMCHA) survey. The survey consisted of a stratified simple random sample of 6,533 adults 18 years of age and older. The strata were the 21 northern counties of the upper-lower peninsula of Michigan participating in the study. Random digit-dialing method was used to generate the probability sample. The sampling frame was all residential telephone numbers ringing into households in the 21 counties that fall into known banks of working residential numbers. The frame was provided by Survey Sampling Incorporated (SSI). The 881 method used all possible area code, exchange, and working block combinations known to contain at least one working household number. As a result, all telephone households in the geographic sampling flame were given equal probability of selection within the limits of available data. A minimum of 300 surveys in each county was required. When a residential number was successfully contacted, the household members 18 years of age and older were listed according to BRFS procedures; 21 men and then women were listed by first name, nickname, or position within family (i.e., father, uncle) fiom oldest to youngest. Wrthin each sample household, an adult respondent was randomly chosen from all resident adults 18 years of age or older. A computer- generated random number was used to select among eligible respondents listed in a household. Substitutions were not permitted (Community Health Assessment, 1995). For this study, in which the focus is on older adults, only subjects aged 55 and older were included in the secondary data analysis. This yielded a sample size of 2,710 (Community Health Assessment, 1995). EieldBmcedures In the primary research study, data were ascertained through a one-time telephone interview conducted by Information Transfer Systems Incorporated (ITS) of Ann Arbor, Michigan, from July to September 1995. ITS, a research firm contracted out by the North Central Counsel of the Michigan Health and Hospital System, followed data collection procedures that are consistent with the Michigan BRFS project (Remington, et al., 1988). ITS used computer-assisted telephone interviewing which permits direct entry of the data into a computer file, thus facilitating interviewer monitoring, data coding and entry, and quality control procedures. Interviewers were ITS employees experienced in health surveys and in the use of computer-assisted interviewing device, and were given specialized training in the use of the survey (Community Health Assessment, 1995). Dmfioflmfimmmdmming When a phone number was called and an eligible respondent selected in the primary research study, 1 of 3 outcomes were possible: (1) the interview was completed; (2) the respondent refilsed to be interviewed or hung up during the interview; or (3) the 22 respondent selected was unavailable and an appointment was made for call-back When the phone mnnber was working but the line was busy, a recorder or no one answered the phone, the number was redialed. Repeated attempts were made to contact the selected respondent. If the respondent was unavailable after multiple attempts, he/she was not included in the sample. Ifthe number reached was identified as a nonresidential number or outside the geographic area studied, the interviewer informed the respondent that he/she was ineligible (Community Health Assessment, 1995). Interviewers used computer-assisted telephoning interviewing devices that displayed the interview instrument on a computer screen and permitted immediate coding of responses by the interviewers, consistent with the state health department data collection procedures. To maintain consistency, information and instructions were given to the subjects using a script. All subjects were asked to respond to a series of questions related to demographics, health behaviors and practices. The questions on the survey were core questions taken from the Michigan BRFS. The core of questions includes one or more questions in the area of general health status, access to health care, smoking, blood cholesterol, diabetes, diet, obesity, hypertension, physical activity, alcohol use, seat belt use, and health preventative practices (Community Health Assessment, 1995). All questions and answers were coded in the primary research study. For this study, in which the purpose is to examine the relationship between SES and behavioral risk factors, only data related to hypertension, smoking, alcohol use, blood cholesterol, obesity, physical activity, income and education was used in the secondary analysis. 23 E . [H E. 1 In the initial study, the principal researcher protected the right of the respondents. Subject participation was voluntary and subjects were free to withdraw from further questioning at any time during the telephone interview. Informed, verbal consent was obtained fi'om each respondent prior to questioning. Names (first name, nickname, or position in family—father, uncle) were only used to randomly select respondents. Confidentiality of subjects was maintained in that respondents were informed that their answers would be kept strictly confidential and no information which identified them would be released or published. Anonymity was maintained in that names were not attached to the individual data collection files which were only accessible to data collectors and researchers. The investigator of the secondary data analysis also protected the rights of subjects through strict adherence to standard criteria set forth by the Michigan State University Committee on Research Involving Human Subjects (Appendix A). Confidentiality and anonymity were preserved in that the investigator did not have access to names and referred to subject’s only by their identification number assigned to the data file. SES, Reported household income fi'om all sources before taxes and highest grade or year of school completed were used as the index of SES which is the independent variable for this study. Income information was obtained fi'om a single questionnaire item that requested total annual income from all sources before taxes in 1994 in the primary research study (item QINC on the survey). Income was recorded in 1 of 5 classes: (1) less 24 . ‘H-l' than $10,000; (2) $10,000 through $19,999; (3) $20,000 through $34,999; (4) $35,000 through $50,000; or (5) greater than $50,000. Education information was obtained fi'om a single questionnaire item that asked highest grade or year of school completed in the primary research study (item QEDUC on the survey). Education was documented in 1 of 4 categories: (1) less than high school; (2) high school graduate; (3) some college; or (4) college graduate (Community Health Assessment, 1995). W), Hypertension was assessed by a single questionnaire item from the primary research survey that asked respondents if they had ever been told by a health professional that they lmve high blood pressure (item Q24 on the survey). Response categories were yes or no. When considered together, two items on the survey were used to measure the variable smoker. Items Q39 and Q41 asked respondents to report: (1) having smoked at least 100 cigarettes in entire life; or (2) smoking cigarettes now, respectively. Response categories for both questions were yes and no. The variable smoker was categorized as “former” and “current;” the former defined by respondents who reported that they had smoked at least 100 cigarettes but no longer smoke, and the latter defined by respondents who reported that they now smoke. Heavy smoker was defined as current smoker who reported smoking 40 or more cigarettes per day. Item Q42 was used to measure heavy smoker. It asked respondents to state number of cigarettes smoke per day on an average. Heavy drinker was defined as consuming 60 or more drinks on an average in the previous month; and binge drinker was defined as 5 or more drinks per occasion consumed at least once in the previous month. Item QALC2 asked respondents how many days per week or month they drank alcoholic beverages. Item QALC3 asked respondents 25 average number of drinks consumed on those days when they did drink. QALC4 asked respondents how many times during the past month did they have five or more drinks on an occasion. These three items when considered together were intended to reflect the variable heavy/binge drinker. Elevated blood cholesterol was defined by a single questionnaire item (item Q27 on the survey) that asked respondents if they had ever been told by a health professional that their blood cholesterol was too high. Overweight was defined as BMI greater than or equal to 27.8 kg/m2 for men and 27.3 kg/m2 for women. BMI will be calculated using item QWGT and QTALL which asked respondents to state their weight and height, respectively. The formula for calculating BMI was weight in kilograms divided by height in meters squared. Inactivity was defined as 2 days per week, or less, of at least moderate exercise, measured using item QMODEX which asked respondents to state average number of days per week that they exercise (Community Health Assessment, 1995). Limitations must be considered when using surveys, computer assisted interviewing, and secondary data. First, telephone surveys may lead to biased population estimates because of the under representation of certain groups. This bias may result from difficulty in contacting certain groups; for example, persons who leave town for long periods of time for work or leisure related reasons; and households without a telephone, often the case in very low income households. Also, this bias may result from high refirsal rates; for example, persons who are unwilling to be surveyed; and severely hearing impaired persons (Remington et al., 1988). Supplementary, telephone surveys, or surveys in general, use a forced-choice answer format. The most appropriate response may not be 26 available to the person being surveyed resulting in inaccurate or missing data. Second, despite the advantages of immediate data entry with computer assisted interviewing, key board errors can occur. Verification of data entry is not possible when data is entered into the computer during the interview. Using paper forms rather than a computer with subsequent key entry would permit detection of obvious errors at the keying stage (Shea, et al., 1991). Third, because the researcher used data collected from a prior research study, questions may be worded or phrased differently than what would have been preferred by the researcher. For example, income was grouped into categories which can be problematic with regards to comparability across households of difi‘erent sizes. Family size must also be included since the impact of a given income is vastly different for a family of 2 compared with a family of 10. Liberatos et al. (1988) recommends using the poverty index developed by the federal government, which adjusts for family size and cost of living, as a base. Income categories that are multiples of the poverty level can be created. The categorical approach is more common since many people feel less uncomfortable if they place themselves into groups. Another example, college graduate was one category choice for measuring education. This category fails to delineate type of college-2 year or 4 year. Type of college, number of years or private verses public; type of degree earned; and major field of concentration are educational differences that in turn can have an impact on subsequent income and occupational attaimnent. Furthermore, the researcher does not have the opportunity to ask additional questions that could prove useful in the secondary data analysis. For example, it is not possible to ascertain from the survey questions measuring the variable smoker, number of 27 years smoked, total amount smoked or “pack years” and smoking cessation date. Additionally, it is not possible to establish from the survey question measuring the variable hypertension as to the respondent’s status-controlled or uncontrolled. These factors need consideration since they afl‘ect rate of risk. Lastly, the results of this study can only be generalizable to the geographic area sampled. The survey was conducted in the summer months which included the seasonal older adults. These older adults are typically more aflluent; thus, the proportion of persons in the lower income groups could potentially be small. Instrumentation The NMCHA survey was administered. The questions for this survey were taken from the BRFS questionnaire which has widespread usage. It is used by more than 40 state health departments in conjunction with the Centers for Disease Control to measure state specific prevalence and time trends of a number of health related behaviors. A core of questions on this questionnaire was derived from other national surveys such as the National Health and Nutrition Examination Surveys and the National Health Interview Survey. The questionnaire was used because of its reliability and validity; and because it concentrates on behavior rather than knowledge or attitudes (Shea, Stein, Lantiqua & Basch, 1991). The BRFS has good test-retest reliability. Stein et al. (1993) administered the survey twice over 21 to 44 days to a representative sample of 210 Massachusetts residents. The prevalence of risk factors was consistent between the two administrations. None of the differences between the two administrations were significant (p > .05). Employing Kappa and Pearson’s r, reliability coeflicients were generally high with most 28 coeficients above .70 and many above .80. For the demographic factor variables, there were no statistically significant differences in the variables across administrations. For education the coeflicient was .80; and for income the coefiicient was .75. Shea et al. (1991) administered the BRFS twice over 10 to 21 days in New York State. Again, the prevalence of each risk factor was consistent between administrations. Reliability coeflicients for the six behavioral risk factors that will be used in this study ranged fi'om .65 to .85. The variable “blood pressure measured in past 2 years” was low but this was not due to discordance in responses between administrations. One respondent was excluded from the variable “current smoker” because on examination of the data, an error was found. A value of 4 was used in the first entry and 74 in the second; presumably a keying error. In the primary research study, responses were recorded according to the scoring key. Respondents who answered that they did not know or refused to answer a question were coded as such. In the secondary analysis, respondents were excluded fi'om the total number for each behavioral risk factor and demographic information if they gave a “did not knouf’ response or refirsed to respond. This approach to item non-response assumes that the proportion of respondents at risk is the same for those with missing information as for those who offer responses to the survey item. This approach also means that the total sample size reported for each questionnaire item will vary slightly. This procedure for handling missing data is different than in the BRFS in that any missing data (respondent answered don’t know or refirsed to answer) for a behavioral risk factor item is viewed as that risk factor being absent. 29 Community Health Assessment (1995) data was stored in Wmdows; file is entitled nccreg.sav. In the secondary analysis, data was analyzed using the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS). Frequency counts for each item were obtained and then reported in percentiles. Remhllesisn This study was a non-experimental descriptive approach selected to examine the relationship between SES and prevalence of behavioral risk factors for stroke among older adults in Northern Michigan. SES, comprising income and education, was the independent variable and 6 behavioral risk factors were the dependent variables. A few statistical procedures were used to analyze the data. First, descriptive statistics were used to summarize the demographic characteristics-particularly, age and gender by income and education Demographic data that was used included income and education which was analyzed categorically by the prevalence of each behavioral risk factor (individual) and the total number of behavioral risk behaviors (aggregated). A tabular presentation (cross-tabulation) of data for prevalence of individual behavioral risk factors for stroke by income and education is in percentage form. Additionally, a tabular presentation of data for prevalence of number of behavioral risk factors by income and education is in percentage form. Distribution of the study population is stratified by age ranges and reported by fiequency to include both men and women by income and education. Second, to test the difference in proportions (prevalence) of individual and total number of behavioral risk factors on income and education level, chi-square statistic was 30 applied to the cross-tabulations. Chi-square is used for testing group differences when the dependent variable(s) is measured on a nominal scale (Polit & Hungler, 1995). The chi- square statistic was used to establish significance of different proportions (prevalence). Statistical significance was set at the .05 level. Two hypotheses were tested in this study. The hypotheses are: (1) higher prevalence of behavioral risk factors—hypertension (HTN), smoking, excessive alcohol consumption, elevated blood cholesterol, obesity, and physical inactivity is associated with lower income; and (2) higher prevalence of behavioral risk factors—HTN, smoking, excessive alcohol consumption, elevated blood cholesterol, obesity, and physical inactivity is associated with lower educational attaimnent. RESULTS The results of this study are reported in three parts: (1) distribution of study population; (2) prevalence of individual and multiple behavioral risk factors; and (3) significance of difi‘erences in proportions of individual and multiple behavioral risk factors. Wins The sample consisted of 2,7 10 adults age 55 and older. Of the 2,710 subjects participating in the study, 1,895 subjects responded to the income level question and 2,701 subjects responded to the education level question. Although there were more subjects in the $20,000 to 34,999 income level and HS graduate education level, all other levels were well represented (Table 1). Income level was less than $10,000 among 15.8% of the study population, $10,000 to $19,999 among 26%, $20,000 to $34,999 among 29.7%, $3 5,000 to $50,000 among 16.7%, and greater than $50,000 among 11.8%. Education level was 31 25.8: 88 can u 5 8v 8” A32 u 5 as new a2: u 5 3m Be 45.2. Gems: E on S 8 v: S 8 920 memdoo $2: an S 3 we 8 m5 2 $348 88m €1.95 £2 5; K as o: 8” e: 3520 mm $5.8 as v: S as m: NS 3 E235 mmmu _ 22:09:”.— _ A38: 32 can u 5 m8 «2 a: u 5 8—. m: a2 u 5 ma. an 45.2. @535 «mm m a 3 a. 8 as Soda A @035 E. R S 9. 8 3 E 25.8-3 @035 8m 2. as N: ”2 a2 a: 833% £5.05 «a. as S .5 ea 3 on amaze; @325 SM 3 8 a R 8 2 Sod; v 3.5 43.3 onB a zmz a 5:53 a 2m: 1. 529$ a 52 a fl HEB:— a, +3 3.3 31?. mm”: _ 035. 32 less than high school (HS) for 23.6% of the subjects participating in the study, HS graduate for 40.1%, some college for 18.8%, and college graduate for 17.5%. E 1 EE 1 . l E. l E The prevalence of individual behavioral risk factors varied by SES and specific behavior. The prevalence of individual behavioral risk factors by income and education is displayed in Table 2. HTN was the most frequently occurring individual risk behavior for the three lower income levels and for all the education levels except college graduate. HTN was most prevalent among subjects with an income level less than $10,000 (51.7%) and education level less than HS (49.3%); and least prevalent among subjects falling into the $3 5,000 to $50,000 income level (42.4%) and college graduate education level (41.8%). Similarly, current smoker, obesity, and inactivity were behaviors most common in the less than $10,000 income level (21.7%, 42.8%, and 30.8%, respectively) and the less than HS education level (19.2%, 42.8%, and 22.4%, respectively). Furthermore, these behaviors were least common in the $50,000 income level for current smoker (11.2%) and $3 5,000 to $50,000 income level for obesity and inactivity (34.9% and 14.2%, respectively) and least common in the college graduate education level ( 10.4%, 30.7%, and 14.3%, respectively). Conversely, heavy smoker, binge and heavy drinker, and elevated cholesterol were behavioral risk factors more common in the greater than $5 0,000 income level (16.0%, 11.7% and 9% and 43.6%, respectively) and college graduate education level (17.0%, 8.1%, 9.7% and 39.8%, respectively) when compared to the lowest income level and the least education level. This unexpected finding could reflect the high price of alcohol, cigarettes, and certain foods high in cholesterol, so that those with the lowest 33 m4; 5.0m mdm 6o mm 95 1: Q? Q3 20 mam—4.50 9w— oém 6; me me $2 5.2 m9. vsv ”yams—400 mEOm n.w_ Nam Q; Wm ad #2 5.2 men 0.2. mH 4 [goons 7. % v. % < $10,000 5.0 23.0 59.3 12.7 81049999 7.7 26.8 50.4 15.0 $20-34,999 8.3 21.8 54.5 15.3 ass-50,000 8.2 21.8 56.5 13.6 > $50,000 7.6 22.4 57.4 12.6 [EDUCATION LESS THAN HS 7.5 24.1 53.3 15.1 HS GRADUATE 8.9 24.0 53.7 13.5 SOME COLLEGE 8.1 24.8 55.2 12.0 COLLEGE GRAD 9.3 24.6 55.3 10.8 35 incomes may find these items too expensive to purchase. The mean age of the individuals with the highest income and education level is unknown. Typically, income drops after retirement and older individuals have less than a college graduate education level. Therefore, it’s plausible that these individuals are still in the workforce and job stressors are responsible for this increase in prevalence of smoking and drinking. Finally, the prevalence of former smoker was positively related to both income and education level; prevalence increased as income and education increased. The prevalence of multiple risk factors varied depending on the SES indicator and total number of behavioral risk factors (Table 3). For all of the income and education levels, subjects reported more often a total of three to four risk factors than any other total mrmber of behavioral risk factors. Additionally, for all of the income and education levels, subjects more often reported a total of greater than four risk behaviors than zero risk behaviors. Among subjects with zero risk factors, fewer were in the lowest income and educational level (5.0% and 7.5%, respectively) as compared to all other levels; and more were in the highest educational level (9.3%) as compared to all other education levels. The prevalence of zero risk factors increased as education level increased; although surprisingly, prevalence was less for some college education level (8.1%) than for HS graduate level (8.9%). The prevalence of 1 to 2 risk factors was, as expected, more likely for the lower income levels (23.0% for less than $10,000 and 26.8% for $10,000 to $19,999) than the higher income levels (21.8% for $35,000 to $50,000 and 22.4% for greater than $50,000); and unexpectedly, less likely for the lower education levels than the higher education levels. Surprisingly, the prevalence of three to four risk factors increased as education and income level increased except in the lowest income level. Furthermore, 36 the end of the income spectrum appear to coincide, the prevalence of greater than four risk factors was similar for the lowest (12.7%) and highest (12.6%) income levels. As expected, the prevalence of greater than four risk factors increased as the education level decreased. Thus, the first hypothesis that higher prevalence of behavioral risk factors-- hypertension (HTN), smoking, excessive alcohol consumption, elevated blood cholesterol, obesity, and physical inactivity is associated with lower income is supported for only some L of the risk behaviors. Namely, hypertension, current smoker, obesity, and inactivity were the risk behaviors more prevalent among the lower income level. Likewise, the second hypothesis that higher prevalence of behavioral risk factors-HTN, smoking, excessive alcohol consumption, elevated blood cholesterol, obesity, and physical inactivity is associated with lower educational attainment is partially supported. The prevalent behavioral risk factors are the same behaviors more prevalent among subjects in the lower income level (HTN, current smoker, obesity, and inactivity). 5° '6 . 1m [E . Chi-Square test was used to examine the difference in proportions (prevalence) of individual and total number of behavioral risk factors for income and education. The chi- square statistic was used to establish significance in different proportions (prevalence). Statistical significance was set at the .05 level. The results of the chi-square for both individual and total number behavioral risk factors are presented in Table 4. The purpose of this study was to answer the question: “Is there a sigmficant relationship between SES and prevalence of behavioral risk factors for stroke among older adults in Northern Michigan?” The results indicate that there were significant differences in prevalence of 37 no. w Am fl. 8. 8. mm. 8. on. 2. 8. 8. 2. $95525 2.. 8. an. S. 8. 8. mu. 8. 8. 2. _ ”—2002: is: 6mm... 93:39:30 .33.... ufibh 85.37: amaze Sausage—6 42504:, mac—3% 2.5 mam EDOmAflU SO >g§m ...u. .. ..u._.o... .. gr." ,,..1. “yoga?“ no ..u n. .W... o . w“... 1.. v 033. 38 some individual behavioral risk factors, but not multiple risk factors, for income and education. There were significant differences in prevalence of former and current smoker (p = .00, p = .01, respectively), binge and heavy drinker (p = .00, p = .00), and inactivity (p = .00) for income level. Similarly, there were significant differences in prevalence of former and wrrent smoker (p = .00, p = .00), heavy drinker (p = .02), obesity (p = .00), and inactivity (p = .00) for education level. Lastly, differences in prevalence of total mrmber of behavioral risk factors for income (p = .46) and education (p = .72) were not significant. I . EE' 1' Conclusions can be drawn fiom the interpretation of the findings of this study with respect to the existing literature and the conceptual framework used in this study. There is a substantial amount of literature on the relationship between SES and individual risk factors; whereas, very little literature is available on SES and multiple risk factors. The literature is even less limited for study population of older adults. By and large, the existing literature supported a higher risk for disease with a decrease in education. Decreased education was related to a higher prevalence of smoking, obesity, hypertension, and inactivity (Winkleby, et al.,1990; Winkleby, et al., 1992; Matthews, et al., 1989; and Pierce, et al., 1989). Prevalence did vary; however, depending on the specific measure used for SES, the specific behavior, and the age group. Additionally, gender was found to be a factor afi‘ecting prevalence with men reporting more drinking and smoking (Cockerman, et al., 1986; Mirand & Welte, 1996). The findings in this study were consistent with previous literature in that the less educated had a higher prevalence of hypertension, current smoking, obesity, and inactivity. The lowest income level also had a 39 higher prevalence of hypertension, current smoking, obesity, and inactivity which is consistent with some literature (Wister, 1996; Gortmaker, et al., 1993; Helrnert, et al., 1989). Findings in this study were inconsistent with an earlier study that found a negative relationship between heavy drinking and SES (Mirand & Welte, 1996). A positive relationship between binge drinking and income, and heavy drinking, income and education was found in this study. Incidently, the risk factor former smoker is more prevalent among the higher income level and higher education level. Although this behavior is considered a risk factor, the rate of risk declines with smoking cessation; hence, subjects in the lower income and education level are at greater risk as former smoker is least prevalent for these levels. This study suggests that among those subjects with a total number of greater than four risk factors for stroke, the rate of risk increases as the education level decreases as prevalence of greater than four risk behaviors is higher with less education. The Paradigm on Research on Socioeconomic Status and Health (Williams, 1990) was used as a fiarnework for this study. Williams’ (1990) paradigm provides an excellent conceptual framework for the variables in this study. The purpose of this study was to determine the extent to which SES is related to the prevalence of behavioral risk factors for stroke. The hypothesized relationship between SES and the health practices listed under psychosocial factors in the fiamework, was partially supported in this research. The prevalence of some of the health practices; precisely, hypertension, current smoker, obesity, and inactivity are clearly related (a negative relationship) to SES. The prevalence of former smoker, and binge and heavy drinking are also clearly related (positive relationship) to SES. It’s noteworthy that the strength of the relationship between SES 4O .833: .8369 83m .53 35:... 885 .EaaB .6 eoeeeoaaom 65.52% .n 8:3... as. 8.2.... - ”88830 530m 2.2. 25 26:82 , ‘ _ _ _ _ _ F a _ _ _ _ _ £2.35 32%.... - 3.26 - .8022er 3.55% .. 539.538 258.: . nae—ea: - 5.33.2.3 .. 80¢er 530m 2 £8on Eoomoaommm sea—8381.58 .. 2:85 - “mafia 03888268 | \ 41 and some of the health practices studied in this research was significant as is suggested by the thick, solid line in the framework. Other health practices studied in this research were found to have a strong association; however, the association was not significant. For this study, medical care (see Figure 1) was not included in the schematic representation of Williams" (1990) paradigm (see Figure 2) as access and utilization of health care were not variables under study. Although this research did not focus on medical care, it is the one area within Williams’ (1990) paradigm where Advanced Practice Nurses can impact health outcomes through a direct, or an interactive, relationship between medical care and psychosocial factors. Subsequently, the schematic representation of Williams’ (1990) paradigm was revised (Figure 3) to include medical care linked directly to psychosocial factors (indicated by a thin, solid line) and as an intermediary between SES and health outcomes (indicated by a dotted line). DISCUSSION I . . . Additional limitations to this study must be acknowledged. First, as with all self- report survey data, over reporting or under reporting of individual behaviors may occur. Recalling of the past, an event or behavior, can be dificult. This is especially true if the events occurred a long time ago, or if they require the recall of separate events or a lot of detailed information as in the survey used in this study (Converse & Presser, 1986). Second, no response bias must be considered when interpreting the findings as they relate to income level. 0f the 2,710 subjects participating in the study, 1,895 subjects responded to the income level question; consequently, there was missing income data on 815 subjects. This can produce misleading results since it is unknown if these 42 nonresponders were of a lower income level. Third, although probability sampling was used in the primary research study, sampling bias must be considered when drawing conclusions in the secondary data analysis. In the primary research study, the 21 counties were the strata; therefore, it was decided to include a minimum of 300 surveys fi'om each county. In the secondary research study, the attributes of interest were income and education which were not identified strata in the sample selection process of the primary research study. Consequently, the middle income group was over represented. The representativeness of the final sample could be enlmnced had the primary researcher determined the proportion of participants needed for the various income and education levels. The primary researcher could have asked prospective subjects questions pertaining to income and education before determining eligibility. Lastly, the literature suggests that risk for stroke increases as the total number of high risk behaviors increases; nonetheless, the inclusion of former smoker in determining prevalence of total number of behavioral risk factors for stroke potentially presents deceiving results. Although this behavior is considered a risk factor for stroke, the rate of risk declines with smoking cessation. No attempt was nude to rank the relative danger posed by each of the behavioral risk factors in this study; however, this would conceivably provide more reliable results on stroke risk rather than considering number only. Former smoker could then be deemed “less dangerous” than the other risk behaviors. This study presents several implications for nurses in advanced practice in primary care settings who are providing health care for older adults in difl‘erent SES groups. The 43 Advanced Practice Nurse (APN) can enhance positive health outcomes through application of Williams’ (1990) paradigm which provides a framework for impacting health practices; consequently, health outcomes. Initially, a distinct understanding of the APN role and responsibilities is a prerequisite in successfirl implementation of interventions for promoting positive health outcomes by the APN. The expert practitioner role provides the base for the integration of role characteristics of the APN. Asanassessor,theAPNcanobtaindataonpersonalandsocialhistory,andpast medical and family history. Social history would include assessing financial status and education level. Assessing medical and family history would be particularly useful in determining risk factors for disease. Additionally, the APN needs to assess individual and family attitudes and behaviors toward health promotion and disease prevention. The nurse can collaborate with the client and family in assessing, improving, enhancing, and evaluating their current health practices. Identification of client risk taking behaviors, providing assistance in decision making to client and family regarding choice of lifestyle, and reinforcement for positive health behavior practices of the client is needed. Health promotion plans for clients are frequently designed with upper SES individuals in mind. For example, recommendations to increase exercise presumes that the client has access to a safe environment where he or she can walk. As a clinician, the APN needs to provide preventative services aggressively targeted at lower SES groups in an effort to reduce disparities in prevalence of risk factors between difl‘erent SES groups. The provision of preventative care will need to be sensitive to SES-related influences on health and health behaviors. Recommendations for adherence and treatment to prevention 44 regimes need to be tailored to the client’s life circumstances. For example, when suggesting exercise, it will be important to explore with clients when and where this will be accomplished, both helping them to overcome real or perceived barriers and to provide opportunities of which they may not be aware. APNs needs to be aware that as SES declines the need for outreach to clients increases because lower SES groups may not seek help voluntarily. Taking part in community health initiatives, such as free clinics, and the development of self-help support groups is a critical role for the APN. Some of the literature suggests that the lower SES group engages in high risk behaviors in an attempt to cope with their stressful life environmental conditions (V enters, 1989; Pesznecker, 1984). Providing concrete resources for problem solving, strengthening and developing social support systems for low income groups can be a mediating factor between SES and health outcome and is therefore an appropriate primary prevention role for the APN. The APN as a counselor, can enhance the coping abilities of clients and ofi‘er social support to clients and families in an efi‘ort to mitigate some of life strains. An idea for an outreach program, is the development of a self-help support group initiated and facilitated by the APN initially, but that eventually could be continued by the group participants themselves. By sharing similar experiences, concrete information about resources, and participating in assisting others with problem solutions, the group members not only receive but also provide support to each other. Consequently, clients of lower SES may be less inclined to utilize ineffective coping strategies such as high risk behaviors as smoking and drinking. As a leader, the primary responsibility of the APN is to the client served, whether the client is an individual, family, group, or community. Since health cannot be separated 45 from its environment as indicated in this study, it is essential that APNs become involved in all aspects of planning for health to maximize the health potential of older adults. This involvement needs to include attention to policy decisions and political action which can be mediating factors between SES and health outcomes. Policy affects the broader aspect of environment, the socioeconomic conditions of homes and communities, as well as the health care delivery environment. Public policy decisions regarding firnding for health and social programs are critical in providing resources and opportunities for low income people. Health care providers have tremendous potential power to influence decision making. The APN needs to get to know politically influential people in the community. Legislators are influenced by the information they receive and the source of that information. It is the responsibility of the APN to become informed. As well-informed, empowered professionals, APNs play a significant role in supporting appropriate legislative initiatives that promote and protect the health of the public and ensuring that preventative health care for low SES groups is given priority. For the most part; however, nurses do not envision themselves as agents for change in state and national politics. Therefore, unquestionably, preparation for this role must begin during nursing education and must be supported by professional nursing organizations and agencies. Successful intervention to reduce the prevalence of behavioral risk factors for stroke associated with lower SES will need to be broad based, addressing not only specific risk factors but also societal conditions that lead to the adoption and maintenance of high risk behaviors. The APN in the educator role needs to provide health education that facilitates an individual’s ability to improve his or her personal living conditions, to make informed decisions about personal, family, and community health practices and to utilize 46 health services appropriately. Often health care education programs are organized by middle SES health care providers based on their own values and attitudes. Even though the programs may be implemented with the lower SES in mind, the special difliculties and differences in how the poor perceive the health care system frequently are not understood or considered. Thus, APNs can solicit the views of the targeted audience (lower SES) and include them in planning and implementation of educational programs. The APN needs to explore new and appropriate techniques and methods to deliver more effective messages h regarding healthy lifestyle behaviors found to prevent acute and chronic disease, decrease h disability, and enhance wellness. It is not merely information distribution, an activity used to increase awareness; rather, it involves guiding persons through stages of problem i solving and decisions making. Finally, education should be developed to help health care providers understand the magnitude of the problem of the SES-high risk behavior relationship and the factors that underlie it. Finding that a variety of unhealthy behaviors chrster not only in the lower, but in the higher SES group, indicates the need for behavioral modification. As a change agent, the APN has the opportunity and the means to modify behavior. Counseling, reinforcement, reminders, and community programs are examples of some efi‘ective strategies for bringing about positive alterations in an individual’s health behavior. As a researcher, the APN can encourage families to participate in research studies in an efl‘ort to firel the progress of future research and to promote positive health outcomes. All professionals who work in primary care and care for older adults should spread the word about the need to consider participating in research studies. APNs because of there close proximity with patients are in an ideal position for raising the issue 47 and encouragm' g and supporting interested clients and their family members. 8 I . E 'E l B I Research that would go beyond merely demonstrating associations with social status is needed. There should be a focus on understanding the behavioral, social, biological, and physiological mediators that link SES and high risk behaviors. Identification and delineation of the mechanisms and processes that link SES to psychosocial factors is necessary. Attention should be paid to aspects of study design and measurement that are critical to determining whether the association is independent of other factors. There is a considerable need to better understand the links between economic policy, health care coverage, unemployment, and other economic phenomena and the prevention and prevalence of high risk behaviors. Measures of SES should be included in all research on high risk behaviors including cross sectional and longitudinal studies. The data should be analyzed categorically by age. It would be useful to know if some unhealthy behaviors are clustered into certain age groups. Findings may suggest that there are important stages within adulthood for attempting to alter health related behaviors. Secondary analysis of existing data sets that contain potential information of SES and high risk behaviors for stroke should be encouraged. Further attention should be devoted to the effect of SES on risk behaviors throughout the life span with a particular focus on clarifying the relative role of SES at various life stages. There is a need to focus on psychosocial and personality factors associated with different SES groups and their relations with high risk behaviors. Moreover, chronic stress associated with work and other daily aspects of life and its role in high risk behaviors needs firrther investigation. 48 Perhaps pilot studies might be undertaken to determine whether modifications of SES are efl‘ective for prevention of high risk behaviors. This may be an arduous task to complete but it could potentially yield significant findings. More observational studies of health practices in relation to stroke risk and intervention studies aimed at modifying behavior related to stroke risk are needed. Lastly, attempts should be made to understand the protective factors that enable some people to stay healthy by avoiding high risk behaviors despite an increased risk due to lower SES. Summary The evidence in support of the importance of SES to health status has been documented through several studies which have found a direct relationship between individual’s SES and their health. Results of this study indicate that SES afl‘ects health behaviors in relatively important ways but this depends on the measure used for SES and the specific high risk behavior. The findings in this study, in part support the existing literature and the hypothesis that higher prevalence of behavioral risk factors is associated with lower income and lower educational attainment. Health care providers in primary care are in an ideal position to effect change in individuals’ behavior by modifying environmental risk factors-income and education—to ultimately promote positive health outcomes. 49 APPENDIX 50 APPENDIX A UCRIHS Approval Letter 51 quuumuwufim| MtNmmSutUMnmm 246 Mammal Building haanuJflmum unmade 517355-21“) FAX:517/432-1171 Imumhwflwflflufl DashWUuwnmmfl fiaunzhknn stauflmwwafin auwmunnflnmumr MICHIGAN STATE U is I \l E ii S I 1‘ Y April 29, 1998 TO: Sharon King _ A110 Life Cienees Bldg. College of NurSing RE: IRE“: 98-295 TITLE: THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN SES AND PREVALENCE OF BEHAVIORAL RISK FACTORS FOR STROKE AMONG OLDER ADULTS REVISION REQUESTED: N/A CATEGORY: l-E APPROVAL DATE: 04/28/98 The University Committee on Research Involving Human Subjects'(UCRIHsl review of this project is complete. I am pleased to adVise that the rights and welfare of the human subjects appear to be adequately rotected and methods to obtain informed consent are apprepriate. Egrefore, the UCRIHS approved this prOJect and any reVisions listed a ve. RENEHAL: UCRIHS approval is valid for one calendar year, beginning with the approval date shown above. Investigators planning to continue a project be and ene year must use the green renewal form (enclosed with t e original agproval letter or when a project is renewed) to seek u te certification. There is a maXimum of four.such expedite renewals ssible. Investigators wishing to continue a prOJect beyond the time need to submit it again or complete reView. REVISIONS: UCRIHS must review an changes in rocedures involving human subjects, rior to in tiation of t e change. If this is done at the time o renewal, please use the reen renewal form. To revise an approved protocol at an 0 her time during the year send your written request to the CRIHS Chair, requesting revised approval and referencing the project's IRB I and title. Include in our request a description of the change and any revised ins ruments, consent forms or advertisements that are applicable. PROBLEMS] CHANGES: Should either of the followin arise during the course of the work, investigators must noti UCRIHS promptly: (1) roblems (unexpected side effects comp aints. e c.) involving uman eubjects or (2) changes the research environment or new information indicating greater risk to the human sub ects than existed when the protocol was previously reviewed an approved. If we can be of any future help, lease do not hesitate to contact us at (517)355-2180 or FAX (517l4 2- 171. sincerely, [Li id 3. Wright, Ph : CRIBS Chair DEW:bed cc: Trudy E. Day 52 BIBLIOGRAPHY 53 BIBLIOGRAPHY American Heart Association, (1988). 81W. Dallas: American Heart Association. Casper, M. ,ng, W. ,Strogatz, D. ,Davis, C. E., & Tyroler, H. A (1992). Antihypertensive treatment and U. S. trends in stroke mortality 1962 to 1980 Amenmlcumalnfhrblicflealfldl 1600-1606 Cassel, J. (1976). The contribution of the social environment to host resistance. Ammcan InumalnfEnideminlmJM, 107-123 Cockerman, W. ,Lueschen, G., Kunz, G. & Spaeth, J. L. (1986). Social stratification and selfmanagemem of health WW, 1-14 Colditz, G. A, Bonita, R, & Starnpfer, M. J. (1988). Cigarette smoking and risk of stroke in middle-aged women .NenLEnglandJnumalnfMedrmneallfi. 937-941 Community Health Assessment. (1995) WW I L'II'HIU 1’31 .Hlllll! l.‘..Ia I "'1'. Id 0 .||.\ Wanna, Ann Arbor: Information Transfer Systems, Inc. Converse, J..,M &Presser, S. (1986). - ' . ' Questinnnaire Newbury Park, California. Sage. Donahue, RP. ,Orchard, T. J., Kuller, L. H., & Drash, AL. (1985). Lipids and lipoproteins rn a young adult population: the Beaver county lipid study. American, InnmalnffinidemialmJZZ, 458-467 Dryer, AR, Starnler, J. Shekelle, R. B., & Schoenberger, J. (1976). The relationship of education to blood pressure: Findings on 40,000 employed Chicagoans. CW, 987-992 Dunbabin, D.W., & Sandercock, PG. (1990). Preventing stroke by the modification of risk factors. W02), Suppl. IV: 36-39. Dyken, M.L., Wolf, P.A., & Barnett, H.J.M. (1984). Risk factors in stroke: A statement for physicians by the subcommittee on risk factors in older women. W 1 105-11 13. Feinstein, J .S. (1993). The relationship between socioeconomic status and health: A review of the literature. W0), 279-321. 54 Feldman, J. J. Makuc, D. M., Kleinman, J. C, & Coroni-Huntley, J. (1989) National trends In educational difl‘erentials 1n mortality. Amencanlmrmalnffimdemmlma 122, 919-933. Ford, E. S. ,Merritt, RK., Health, G. W. ,Powell, K. E. ,Washburn, RA, Kriska, A, & Haile, G. (1991). Physical activity behaviors in lower and higher socioeconomic status populations. AmedeanlmnnalnffipidemialoqulUZ), 1246-1256. Gortmaker, S.L., Must, A, Perrin, J.M., Sobel, A.M., & Dietz, W.H. (1993). Social and economic consequences of overweight in adolescence and young adulthood. NearEnghndloumalnfMedicinelZQ 1008-1012- Haheim, L., Holrne, 1., Hjermann, 1., & Leren, P. (1993). Risk factors for stroke incidence and mortality: A 12-year follow-up of the 0510 study. W10), 1484-1489. Helrnert, U., Herman, B., Joeckel, K.H., Greiser, E., & Madans, J. (1989). Social class and risk factors for coronary heart disease in the Federal Republic of Germany: Results of the baseline survey of the German cardiovascular prevention study. WM 37-42 Hillbom, M. & Kaste, M. (1982). Alcohol intoxication: A risk factor for primary subarachnoid hemorrhage. W, 706-709. Holrne, 1., Helgeland, A, Hjermann, 1., Leren, P., & Lund-Larsen, PG. (1981). Physical activity at work and at leisure in relation to coronary risk factors and social class: A 4-year mortality follow-up. 209, 277-283. Iriberren, C., Russell, R. V. ,,Luepker R. ,McGovern, P. G. ,Arnett, D. K. & Blackburn, H. (1997). Twelve-Year trends in Cardiovascular disease risk factors in the Minnesota heart survey AmhiyeslnternalMedicineJfl, 873-881 Iso, H, Jacobs, D. R, & Wentworth, D. (1989). Serum cholesterol levels and six-year mortality fi'om stroke in 350, 977 men screened for the multiple risk factor intervention trial WWW, 904-910 Jacobs, D. R, McGovern, P. G. & Blackburn, H. (1992). The U. S. declines in stroke mortality. What does ecological analysis tell us? WM W,1596-1599. Jackson, C. ,Jatulis, D. E. ,& Fortrnarr, S. P. (1992). The behavioral risk factor survey and the Stanford five-city project survey. A comparison of cardiovascular risk behavior estimates W6) 412-416 Jacobsen, B. K. ,& Thelle, D. S. (1988). Risk factors for coronary heart disease and level of education Americanloumalnffipidemiologx, 923-932 55 Jefl‘ery, RW. & French, S. A (1996). Socioeconomic status and weight control practices among 20 to 45 year old women Ammlamflnfhrhhmmhhm?) 1005- 1010 Kaplan, G.A, & Keil, J.E. (1993). Socioeconomic factors and cardiovascular disease: A review ofthe literature. WM), 1973-1998. Keil, J. E., Sutheriand, S. E. ,Knapp, R G, & Tyroler, H A (1992) Does equal socioeconomic status in black and white men equal risk of mortality? Ameriean, W2, 1133- 1136 Keil, J. E ,,Tyroler H. A, Sandifer, S. H., & Boyle, E. (1977). Hypertension: Efl'ects of social class and racial admixture the results of a cohort study m the black pontilation of Charleston, South Carolina Amefimnloumalnfhrhfieflealthfl, 634-639 Keil, J.E., Sandifer, S. H., Loadholt, C. B., & Boyle, E. (1981). Skin colorand education efi‘ects on blood pressure. WWW], 532-534. Kelly-Hayes, M. (1991). A preventative approach to stroke. Wmeeeflflmh W4). 931-942. Kleg, M. J., & Whelton, P. K, (1987). Risk of stroke in male cigarette smokers. Neg M. 628-634 Kessler, R C. (1979). Stress, social status, and psychological distress. Jenmalefflmlth, Winona), 259-272 Liberatos, P. ,Link, B. G., & Kelsey, J. L. (1988). The measurement of social class in epidemiology Epidemialesielleumsfl, 87-121 Matcher, D. ,McCrory, D. ,Bamett, H ,& Feussner, J. (1994). Medical treatment for stroke Prevention Ammmfiofleamfflhimmlm 1) 41- 53 Matthews K. A, Kelsey, S. F. ,Meilalrn, E. N., Kuller, L H. & W'mg, RR (1989). Educational attainment and behavioral and biological risk factors for coronary heart disease In middle aged women Amefieanlmrmalnffipidemiolmnflo), 1 132-1 144. Michigan Department of Public Health (1993). W Michigan Department of Health. Mirand, AL. ,& Welte, J. W. (1996). Alcohol consumption among the elderly m a general POPuletion, Ene County New York Ameneanlaumalnfhrhheflealthfiflfl 978-984. 56 National Center for Health Statistics (1990). Henlthflnitedfitnes. Publ No. (PHS) 91- 1232. Hyattsville, MD:US Government Printing Ofice. Nelson, M A (1994) Economic impoverishment as a health risk: Methodologic and conceptual issues W96) 1 12 Oken, B. ,Hartz, A, Giefer, E. ,& Rimm, AA (1977). Relation between socioeconomic status and obesity changes in 9046 women. BREW 447-453. Palomaki, H., & Kaste, M. (1993). Regular light-to-moderate intake of alcohol and the risk of ischemic stroke: Is there a beneficial effect? W12), 1828-1832. Peszencker, BL. (1984). The poor". A population at risk. WW, 237-249). Pierce, J. P., Fiore, M. C. ,Novotny, T. E. ,Hatziandreu E. J., & Davis, RM. (1989). Trends in cigarette smoking in the United States: Projections to the year 2000., WWW, 61-65. Polit, D.F., & Hungler, B.P. (1995). t w 1.‘ ' : Philadelphia: J .B. Lippincott. Remington, PL, Smith, M.Y., Williamson, D.F., Anda, RF., Gentry, E.M., & Hogelin, GO. (1988). Design, characteristics, and usefiilness of state-based behavioral risk factor surveillance: 1981-87. WM), 366-375. Roget, E., Sorlie, P. D. ,Johnson, N. J., & Scmitt, (1992) A mortality study of 1. 3 million persons by demographic, social, and economic factors: 1979-1985 follow-up. WW NIH Publication No.92-3297,1-5. Salonen, J. T. (1982). Socioeconomic status and risk of cancer, cerebral stroke, and death due to coronary heart disease and any disease: A longitudinal study in eastern Finland. Iaumalnffimdemiolosicalflnmmunityflmlthilfi, 294-297 Salonen, J. T., Slater, J. S. ,,Tuornilehto J. ,Rauramaa, R (1988). leisure time and occupational physical activity: Risk of death from ischemic heart disease. Amefimlmrmalnffipidmiolosyilu 87-94 Shea, S. Stein, A D., Lantiqua, R, & Basch, C E. (1991). Reliability of the behavioral risk factor survey in a triethnic population. WWW 131(5) 489-500. Siegel, D., Kuller, L., Lazarus, N.B., Black, D., Feigal, D., Hughes, G., Schoenberger, J. A, & Hulley, S. B (1987). Predictors of cardiovascular events and mortality in the systolic hypertension in the elderly program pilot project. Ameneanleumalef W, 385-399 57 Slater, C. ,& Carlton, B. (1985). Behavior, lifestyle, and socioeconomic variables as determinants of health status: Implications for health policy development. AmedmloumalnffirsxenfixeMedieineJfi), 25-33 Stein, A. D. ,Lederman, RI, & Shea, S. (1993) The behavioral risk factor surveillance system questionnaire: Its reliability in a statewide sample. Amerieanlenmalef WU” 1763-1772 Uphold, CR. & Graham. M-V- (1994). WWW). Gainesville, Florida: Barmarrae Books. US. Department of Health and Human Services, Public Health Service. Henlthyfieenle, 20%). National Health Promotion and Disease Prevention Objectives, Publ. No. (PHS) 91-50213. Washington, DC Government Printing Office, 1990. U. S. Preventive Services Task Force (1996). Screening for asymptomatic carotid artery stenosis WWW Baltimore: Williams & Wilkins. Venters, M. H. (1989). F amrly-onented prevention of cardiovascular disease: A social epidemiological approach. W“) 309-314 Wannarnethee, S.G., & Shaper, AG. (1996). Patterns of alcohol intake and risk of stroke in middle-aged British men. Wfi), 1033-1039. Williams, D. R. (1990) Socioeconomic difl‘erentials rn health: A review and redirection. W0) 81-99 Winkleby, M. A Fortman, S. P., & Barrett, D. C. (1990) Social class disparities in risk factors for disease: Eight-year prevalence patterns by level of education. WW1) 1- 12 kaleby, M., Jatulis, D. ,Frank, E. ,& Fortman, S. (1992). Socioeconomic status and health: How education, income, and occupation contribute to risk factors for cardiovascular disease .Ameficanlaumalnfhrblieflealtuzw) 816-820 Wister, A. V. (1996). The effects of socioeconomic status on exercise and smoking. WWW 467-488 Wolf, P.A, D’Agostino, R.B., Belanger, AJ., & Kannel, W.B. (1991). Probability of stroke: A risk profile fi'om the Framingham study. W0), 312-318. Wolf, RA (1990). An overview of the epidemiology of stroke. W102), Suppl H: 4-6. 58 "llllllllllllilli