.m . .m L. 2.5.; . . s... .0 . ... 2.; . . I). .T...r...,._li _u.. J . am» a. 2.16....” . .9. .. . w.) 2 .fiuyw.n§m.fiwu&m.p t .... 5 .... 1 . . r“ 1.21.... .3: . a... 31. Hurrah-n. why-mm. . a... I; ...-.... .3» fig?! v . -. flew-n. .«uvi $.52. ’3: b .0}! .. ha_.u...y...:.mw-fll 1 . ”..., . . @1237 HEW»??? 'I .- .l ! [(29.5]..ka - - . 99!? 3.3%.0ICWI'JE‘U7 AW . . ._ - . .-.. 2...... 11.- 2.... 3333.23“. 3......” 11,32?" 2, V??? LIBP 1‘ W I WWW“I“\“Hililil‘ilil\IWIHHHIHN Michigan mm..- 293 083 University This is to certify that the dissertation entitled ADJUSTMENT TO COLLEGE: THE CONTRIBUTION OF A LIVING-LEARNING PROGRAM FOR SCIENCE AND ENGINEERING STUDENTS presented by Cynthia K. Helman has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for Ph.D. degree in Educational Administration l -. X/W Wm, a r professor Date 3/23/99 MS U is an Affirmative Action/Equal Opportunity Institution 0- 12771 PLACE IN REI'URN BOX to remove this checkout from your record. TO AVOID FINES return on or before date due. MAY BE RECALLED with earlier due date if requested. I DATE DUE DATE DUE DATE DUE -. 3N 12 9 2005 'ECM 9 2006 1/” WWW“ ADJUSTIVIENT TO COLLEGE: THE CONTRIBUTION OF A LIVING-LEARNING PROGRAM FOR SCIENCE AND ENGINEERING STUDENTS By Cynthia K. Helman A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University In partial fulfillment of the requirements For the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Educational Administration 1999 ABSTRACT ADJUSTMENT TO COLLEGE: THE CONTRIBUTION OF A LIVING-LEARNING PROGRAM FOR SCIENCE AND ENGINEERING STUDENTS By Cynthia K. Helman This study examined the contribution of a residential program on adjustment to college and fall semester grade point average for 174 first year science and engineering students. Specifically, social and academic aspects of the living-learning program were examined to determine their relationship with academic adjustment, social adj ustment, full adjustment, and fall semester grade point average. The Student Adaptation to College Questionnaire was used to measure student adjustment to college, and a survey designed for this study was used to measure students' levels of involvement with the academic and social aspects of the program. The data were analyzed using t-tests and multiple regression. None of the academic or social aspects directly related to the living-leaming program were identified by multiple regression as significant predictors of adjustment or fall GPA. Significant predictors of academic adjustment were knowing one's roommate prior to college attendance and the grade received for the seminar class required as a part of the living—leaming program. The number of hours per week students spent in class and being satisfied with the roommate relationship were significant predictors of social adjustment. Significant predictors of full adjustment were knowing one's roommate prior to college attendance, the number of hours per week spent in class, and being female. Predicted grade point average and the grade for the seminar class were significant predictors of fall semester grade point average. Limitations of the study were discussed with implications for both further research and professional practice. TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES ............................................................................................. iii LIST OF FIGURES ............................................................................................. v CHAPTER 1 INTRODUCTION Introduction and Statement of the Problem .......................................................... 1 Purpose of the Study ............................................................................................ 8 Research Questions ............................................................................................. 8 Methodology ....................................................................................................... 9 Significance of the Study ................................................................................... 10 Limitations ........................................................................................................ 1 1 Organization of the Dissertation ........................................................................ 12 CHAPTER 2 REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE Introduction ....................................................................................................... 1 3 Attrition from Higher Education ........................................................................ 13 Tinto’s Model of Longitudinal Departure .......................................................... 18 Transition to College ......................................................................................... 22 Adjustment to College ....................................................................................... 23 Research about Student Adjustment to College .................................................. 25 Summary ........................................................................................................... 33 CHAPTER 3 METHODOLOGY Participants ........................................................................................................ 37 Instruments ........................................................................................................ 38 Data Collection .................................................................................................. 43 Data Analysis .................................................................................................... 44 Hypotheses ........................................................................................................ 44 CHAPTER 4 FINDINGS Description of the Sample .................................................................................. 46 Adjustment Scores for Respondents .................................................................. 48 Fall Semester Grade Point Averages for Respondents ........................................ 52 ROSES Experiences Survey Data for Respondents ............................................ 55 Results .............................................................................................................. 62 Academic Adjustment ....................................................................................... 63 Social Adjustment ............................................................................................. 68 Full Adjustment ................................................................................................. 73 Fall Semester Grade Point Average ................................................................... 79 Summary of Bivariate Analyses ......................................................................... 84 Multiple Regression Analysis ............................................................................ 86 Academic Adjustment ....................................................................................... 89 Social Adjustment ............................................................................................. 90 Full Adjustment ................................................................................................. 91 Fall Semester Grade Point Average ................................................................... 92 Summary ........................................................................................................... 93 CHAPTER 5 SUMMARY, DISCUSSION, AND RECOMMENDATIONS Summary and Discussion .................................................................................. 97 Implications for Practice ................................................................................... 106 Limitations of the Study ................................................................................... 109 Recommendations for Further Research ........................................................... 1 10 APPENDICES Appendix A: Science and Engineering Baccalaureate Degree Programs Michigan State University .......................................................... 113 Appendix B: Survey Item Clusters .................................................................... 115 Appendix C: ROSES Experiences Survey ......................................................... 118 Appendix D: Correlation Matrix for Variables and Outcomes ........................... 123 Appendix E: UCRIHS Approval Letter ............................................................ 124 Appendix F: Letter to Western Psychological Services ..................................... 125 Appendix G: Letter to ROSES Students ........................................................... 126 BIBLIOGRAPHY ............................................................................................ 127 LIST OF TABLES Table 1 - Components and Measures of ROSES Experiences ............................. 42 Table 2 - Distribution of Respondents by Gender, Ethnicity, and College of Enrollment ..................................................................................... 47 Table 3 - Analysis of Variance of Predicted GPA by Minority Group ................. 48 Table 4 - Descriptive Statistics for Adjustment Scores ....................................... 49 Table 5 - Correlation Among Academic, Social, and Full Adjustment ............... 49 Table 6 - T-tests of Means of Adjustment Scores by Gender .............................. 50 Table 7 - T-Tests of Means of Adjustment Scores by Ethnicity .......................... 50 Table 8 - T-Tests of Means of Adjustment Scores by College ........................... 51 Table 9 - T-Tests of Means of Adjustment Scores by Predicted Grade Point Average .......................................................................... 52 Table 10 - T-Tests of Means for Grade Point Averages by Gender .................... 53 Table 11 — T-Tests of Means for Grade Point Averages by Ethnicity .................. 53 Table 12 — T-Tests of Means for Grade Point Averages by College .................... 54 Table 13 - T-Tests for Fall Semester Grade Point Averages by Predicted GPA .................................................................................. 55 Table 14 - Descriptive Statistics for ROSES Components .................................. 57 Table 15 - Means of ROSES Components by Gender ........................................ 58 Table 16 - Means of ROSES Components by Ethnicity ...................................... 59 Table 17 - Means of ROSES Components by College ........................................ 60 Table 18 - Means of ROSES Components by Predicted GPA ............................. 61 Table 19 - T-Tests for Participation with Academic Components and Academic Adjustment ................................................................ 65 Table 20 - T-Tests for Participation with Social Components and Academic Adjustment ................................................................ 68 Table 21 - T-Tests for Participation with Social Components and Social Adjustment ...................................................................... 71 Table 22 - T-Tests for Participation with Academic Components and Social Adjustment ...................................................................... 73 Table 23 - T-tests for Participation with Academic Components and Full Adjustment ......................................................................... 76 Table 24 - T-tests for Participation with Social Components and Full Adjustment ......................................................................... 79 Table 25 - T-tests for Participation with Academic Components and Fall Semester GPA ..................................................................... 81 Table 26 - T-tests for Participation with Social Components and Fall Semester GPA ..................................................................... 84 Table 27 - Summary of Relationships Between Components and Outcomes .................................................................................. 86 Table 28 - Regression Coefficients for Model Predicting Academic Adjustment ...................................................................... 90 Table 29- Regression Coefficients for Model Predicting Social Adjustment ............................................................................ 91 Table 30 - Regression Coefficients for Model Predicting Full Adjustment ............................................................................... 92 Table 31 - Regression Coefficients for Model Predicting Fall GPA .................... 93 Table 32 - Summary of Relationships Between Predictors and Outcomes .......... 96 Table 33 — Correlation Matrix for Variables and Outcomes ............................... 123 LIST OF FIGURES Figure 1 — Tinto’s Model of Longitudinal Departure ......................................... 20 Figure 2 -— The Contribution of a Living—Learning Program on Academic Adjustment, Social Adjustment, and Fall GPA ................ 34 Chapter 1 INTRODUCTION AND STATEMENT OF THE PROBLEM Going to College Leaving home to attend college can be a very exciting time. For some students, going to college is the next logical and expected step after high school. For other students, college attendance signifies a somewhat uncomfortable departure from the usual norms and expectations of their communities and families. For all students, however, the first few months of college is a time of transition as students manage the separation from previous relationships and communities and begin to explore the norms and expectations of the new culture. It is during this time that new students seek to learn the unique intellectual and social behaviors of the college environment. The transition from high school or work to college is an exceedingly complex phenomenon. The nature and dynamics of the process vary according to the student's social, family, and educational background; personality; educational and occupational orientations and aspirations; the nature and mission of the institution being attended; the kinds of peers, faculty, and staff members encountered; the purpose and nature of those encounters, and the interactions of all these variables. The process is a highly interrelated, web-like series of interpersonal, academic, and organizational pulls and pushes that shape student learning (broadly conceived) and persistence. (Terenzini, Allison, Millar, Rendon, Upcrafi, Gregg, & Jalomo, 1992, p. 39-40) Research supports that the first year of college is a critical time for new student adjustment (Christie & Dinham, 1991; Noel, 1985; Tinto, 1993; Upcraft & Gardner, 1989). Tinto (1993) cites data from the American College Testing Program (ACT) for the fall 1990 entering class in which first-year attrition from four-year institutions was an average of 26.8%. Of all institutional departures from four-year institutions, 53.3% occur during or afier the first year. Much of the research suggests that the first six weeks are the most important time for new students. There is evidence that experiences in this early period determine students’ likelihood of persistence to the second semester and beyond (Nelson, Scott, & Bryan, 1984; Noel, 1985; Tinto, 1988). In the transition to college, many students experience confusion, ambiguity, and uncertainty. The resulting stress may be overwhelming for some and lead to their withdrawal from college. Tinto (1993) states: Though most students are able to cope with the problems of transition, many voluntarily withdraw from college very early in their first academic year, less from an inability to become incorporated in the social and academic communities of the college than from an inability to withstand the stresses that such transitions commonly induce. (p. 98) The focus of this study is student adjustment to college as situated in the research on college student attrition. The theoretical perspective that is most instructive is the longitudinal model of college student departure as proposed by Tinto (1975). Van Gennep’s (1960) framework of transitions is also helpful in highlighting the importance of the adjustment process in becoming integrated into a new environment. A review of college student adjustment research presents the ways in which others have examined student adjustment to college. Finally, the role of institutional strategies in promoting student adjustment to college will be explored. Tinto’s Longitudinal Theory of Departure Tinto (1975) supported the work of Spady (1970; 1971) in asserting the importance of both the academic and social systems of the university in the assimilation or integration of students into the environment. Although Tinto recognized pre-entry characteristics such as prior academic achievement and family background as factors in student persistence, he focused on what happened to students after they entered college. Tinto (1975) suggested that the process of dropout fi'om college can be viewed as a longitudinal process of interactions between the individual and the academic and social systems of the college during which a person's experiences in those systems (as measured by his normative and structural integration) continually modify his goal and institutional commitments in ways which lead to persistence and/or varying forms of dropout. (p. 94) His model emphasizes the distinction between students who are academically dismissed by the institution and those who voluntary leave the institution, stressing that the majority of students leave college for non-academic reasons. Less than 25 % of all institutional departures, nationally, take the form of academic dismissal. Most departures are voluntary in the sense that they occur without any formal compulsion on the part of the institution. Rather than mirroring academic difficulties, they reflect the character of the individual's social and intellectual experiences within the institution. Generally, the more satisfying those experiences are felt to be, the more likely are individuals to persist until degree completion. Conversely, the less integrative they are, the more likely are individuals to withdraw voluntarily prior to degree completion. (Tinto, 1993, pp. 49-50) Transition to College For many adolescents, going to college is a major life transition. This transition can be described as simultaneously exhilarating and anxiety-producing; exciting and overwhelming; stimulating and confiJsing. Feldman and Newcomb (1969) described the new student in college as a novice in an unfamiliar social organization, and therefore confronted with the values, norms, and role stnrctures of a new social system and various new subsystems. Such an experience usually involves desocialization (pressures to unlearn certain past values, attitudes, and behavior patterns) as well as socialization (pressures to learn the new culture and participate in the new social structure). (p. 89) All students are challenged by new academic demands; and most students experience new social situations, a sense of increased independence, and greater freedom than they had in their previous environment. Those who live in residence halls are also required to negotiate new peer relationships in an environment quite unlike any they have previously encountered. For many, this is the first time they will share a room with any other person; let alone with a person from a different background or with values different from their own. The transition to college can be related to the framework proposed by van Gennep (1960) in his famous work, Rites of Passage. Essentially, van Gennep suggested that individuals who move from one social situation to another, or fiom one culture to another, go through a process involving three phases: separation, transition, and incorporation. Tinto (1988) proposed that individuals moving from high school to college go through the same three phases. Student Adjustment to College According to Baker (1989), adjustment to college is the process of coping with the multifaceted demands of the new environment. Adjustment is characterized by separating from the past, learning new norms and behaviors, and becoming a member of the new environment. This period is often accompanied by feelings of uncertainty, confusion, and normlessness. During their first few weeks of college life, freshmen see themselves (and perhaps more important, are seen by host students, faculty members, and administrators) more as nameless, faceless, members of a single undifferentiated social category than at any subsequent time in their college careers. (Wallace, 1966, p. 94) Student adjustment to college has been associated with selecting an academic major (Baker & Siryk, 1986; Smith & Baker, 1987); internal academic locus of control and self-esteem (Mooney, Sherman, & LoPresto, 1991); having positive relationships with parents (Lopez, 1991; Rice, 1992); and having parents whose marriage is not distressed or conflicted (Lopez, Campbell, & Watkins, 1988). Cooper & Robinson (1988) have suggested that adjustment to college is likely to be related to factors not only within an individual, but also in the institutional environment. Social effectiveness (Baker & Siryk, 1983; Christie & Dinham, 1991); alienation (Baker & Siryk, 1980); and social and intellectual validation (Murguia, Padilla, & Pavel, 1991; Stoecker, Pascarella, & Wolfle, 1988; Terenzini et al., 1996) are related to experiences with others in the environment which influence new students’ adjustment to college. Institutional Strategies Colleges and universities have used the research about attrition to develop policies and strategies to reduce student departure and to assist students in their transition to college. At some institutions, elaborate predictive models have been developed to guide admissions decisions. Other institutions have designed retention strategies for specific groups of students or for the entire campus (Noel, 1985). Orientation programs have been improved or expanded to last an entire academic term. Orientation Programs Orientation programs are one effective strategy many institutions use to introduce new students to college life. Students attending orientation have been found to have significantly higher levels of social integration and commitment to the institution than students not attending orientation, even when background characteristics were taken into account (Pascarella, Terenzini, & Wolfle, 1986). Extended orientation programs have taken the form of seminars which may last from several weeks of the first academic term to the entire semester. These programs include freshmen seminars, University 101 courses, and College Success seminars. These programs typically include exercises to promote social interaction, topics such as time management and study skills, an introduction to support resources, and some exploration of academic or career topics (Jewler, 1989). Tinto (1988) suggested that several other kinds of activities (e.g., fraternities, sororities, extracurricular programs) " may all serve to provide individuals with opportunities to establish repetitive contact with other members of the institution in circumstances which lead to the possibility of integration" (Tinto, 1988, p. 446). Residential Programs Much research has focused on the relationship between living in residence halls and college outcomes (see Blimling, 1993; Pascarella, 1991). Living in a residence hall during the first year of college has been found to relate significantly to positive outcomes such as persistence, as well as involvement on campus, gains in interpersonal skills, attitudes, and values (Astin, 1977; Chickering, 1975). Because of the apparent value of living in a residence hall on campus, institutions with such facilities might want to study ways to enhance the normal positive effect of residential living. Such studies should include an assessment of the effects of roommates, peer groups, living-study arrangements in the residence halls, programming, and staffing of facilities. The results might well put institutions in a position to capitalize still more on the potential value of the residence hall experience. (Astin, 1977,p.160) Nationally, in the last 20 years, a variety of residential programs have been designed and implemented with the common purpose of assisting students’ academic and social adjustment to college. For example, living-learning centers were a hallmark of the 1970’s in student housing. New residence halls were built to include classroom space, faculty office space, libraries, and other “academic” components. These centers provided students with opportunities to take classes, interact with faculty, develop relationships with students in the same field of study, and participate in co-curricular activities which complemented their academic work. Planned activities integrating curricular and co- curricular aspects were necessary to achieve maximum benefit from living-learning centers. Renewed emphasis on undergraduate education on many campuses in the 19903 has resulted in revitalized efforts to integrate the academic and social components within the residential environment. In addition, national studies within the past several years have documented the decline in the number of college students who complete baccalaureate degrees in science and engineering (Atkinson, 1990). The Residential Option for Science and Engineering Students (ROSES) at Michigan State University was developed in 1993 and targets entering students intending to major in a science or engineering field. The program provides academic and social experiences and is intended to assist students in their transition to college and to enhance the retention of students in science and engineering majors. All students in the ROSES program live in the same residence hall. They are required to take a one-credit seminar during the fall semester, which includes a weekly class and co-curricular activities. Faculty and academic staff have primary responsibility for the weekly class session, while the residence life staff implement co-curricular seminars and activities which complement the class. Most ROSES students take at least one other course with other ROSES students. There are tutoring services and special tutoring and resource rooms in the residence hall. Additionally, academic advisers hold periodic advising sessions in the residence hall. Purpose of the Study The purpose of this study was to determine the relative contribution of the individual components (required course, relationships with peers, relationships with academic advisers and instructors, involvement) of the ROSES program in predicting student adjustment to college. The Fall 1997 cohort of ROSES students were the participants in the study. Their levels of involvement and participation with the various components of the ROSES program were examined in relation to their academic, social, and fill] adjustment to college, as well as their fall semester grade point average. Research Questions This study primarily addressed the question of whether components of a semester- long program combining academic and residential experiences enhance student adjustment to college. Specifically, this study sought to determine the components of the residential program (ROSES) that best predicted academic adjustment, social adjustment, full adjustment, and fall semester grade point average. This study attempted to answer the following questions: 1. Do specific components of the ROSES program contribute differentially to academic adjustment, social adjustment, full adjustment, and fall semester grade point average? 2. Which components of the ROSES program make the most significant contributions in predicting academic adjustment, social adjustment, firll adjustment, and fall semester grade point average? Methodology Instruments The Student Adaptation to College Questionnaire (Baker & Siryk, 1989) was used in this study. The Student Adaptation to College Questionnaire (SACQ) asks respondents to reflect on their adjustment once they are in college. This instrument yields an overall adjustment score, as well as subscale scores on four facets of adjustment (academic adjustment, social adjustment, personal-emotional adjustment, and attachment). The academic and social adjustment subscales were used in this study because the subscales parallel the academic and social integration components identified by Tinto’s theoretical model. The full adjustment score was also examined. Several studies have supported the use of the SACQ as a valid and reliable instrument for measuring college student adjustment (Baker, 1986; Baker & Siryk, 1986; Cooper & Robinson, 1988; Dahmus, Bemardin, & Bemardin, 1992; Krotseng, 1992; Mooney et. al., 1991; Smith & Baker, 1987) Participants were also asked to complete a brief survey about their experiences during the fall semester. The ROSES Experiences Survey, designed specifically for this study, assessed study habits, interactions with academic and student affairs staff, relationships with student peers, and perceptions of the ROSES program. Collection of Data The Student Adaptation to College Questionnaire (SACQ) and the ROSES Experiences Survey were administered to all students enrolled in the ROSES program during the last two weeks of fall semester, 1997. Surveys were distributed via student mailboxes in the residence hall for students in the College of Natural Science and the College of Agriculture and Natural Resources. Students in the College of Engineering were participating in another study that incorporated this study; thus, the surveys were distributed during the seminar class. Demographic data (gender, ethnicity, and college of enrollment), predicted grade point average, ROSES seminar grades, and fall semester grade point average were obtained from institutional records. Data Analysis Relationships between individual components of the ROSES program and two sets of outcomes—adjustment to college (academic, social, and fill] adjustment) and fall semester grade point average—were examined. Regression analysis was utilized to identify significant predictors of academic adjustment, social adjustment, firll adjustment, and fall semester grade point average. Significance of the Study This study contributes to the literature about institutional strategies which may influence student adjustment to college. Specifically, the focus of the study was a residential program for entering science and engineering students and its contribution to student adjustment. The results of this study may be most significant to science and engineering students who are entering Michigan State University as they consider 10 programs and services which might assist them in meeting their educational goals. Students may elect to participate in programs similar to the ROSES program because of measurable factors which can be communicated to prospective students. These findings may also be of interest to those concerned about the number of prospective scientists and engineers. Institutional leaders and program designers may also be interested in these findings for the improvement of strategies to assist new students. Student retention remains a topic of interest on most campuses. It is estimated that 85% of college student attrition is voluntary (Tinto, 1985); that is, not as a result of an institutional action due to poor academic performance. Adjustment to college is a factor in academic and social integration, which are positively related to goal and institutional commitment; in turn, they are positively related to retention. Institutions have implemented many types of programs designed to assist with student adjustment to college. A residential program which integrates academic and social components may be a particularly viable strategy. On a local level, the results of this study are of interest to the faculty and staff who have designed and implemented the ROSES program since fall, 1993. In addition, replications or adaptations of this residential program could be implemented by other disciplines to improve adjustment and contribute to retention. Students, institutional decision-makers, and program designers may benefit from the results of this study. Limitations Findings from this study have limited generalizability. Because the study was conducted at only one institution, results are not generalizable to other institutions. Only students expressing a preference for science or engineering majors were included; 11 therefore, this study may not be generalizable to students in other fields of study. This study only examined science and engineering majors in one first-year student class. Each first-year student class may differ from one another; thus, caution must be exercised in applying the findings of this study to subsequent groups of first-year science and engineering students. Another limitation is that the residential experiences of all students in this study are only one part of the complex set of college experiences. While this study focused exclusively on the experiences associated with participation in the residential program, it is acknowledged that experiences beyond the scope of this study may influence adjustment to college. Organization of the Dissertation The dissertation is organized into five chapters with the addition of appendices. The second chapter presents a review of the literature relative to student departure, stages of transition, and adjustment to college. The design and methodology used in collecting and analyzing the data is provided in chapter three. Analysis of the data is presented in chapter four. Chapter five includes a summary of the research findings, implications, and recommendations for future research. 12 Chapter 2 REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE Introduction In this chapter, a review of literature related to college student adjustment is presented. Adjustment to college is an important factor in college student persistence; therefore, the first section provides a brief summary of student attrition literature. The second section introduces Tinto’s (1975) model of longitudinal departure, with particular attention to the importance of academic and social integration. Third, van Gennep’s (1960) description of “rites of passage” is examined relative to student transition to college. The fourth section provides a review of research about student adjustment to college. Attrition from Higher Education College student attrition is a concern affecting institutions throughout the country. All colleges and universities regardless of size, type, or geographical location experience student attrition. "Of the nearly 2.4 million students who in 1993 entered higher education for the ISI time, over 1.5 million will leave their first institution without receiving a degree. Of those, approximately 1.1 million will leave higher education altogether, without ever completing either a 2 or 4 year degree program.” (Tinto, 1993). Levine (1989) suggested that less than half of the students enrolled in college graduate in four years and less than 70% graduate in seven years. Perhaps more disturbing is the fact that a large majority of all institutional departures are voluntary. That is, from the institution’s perspective, students are eligible to remain. "Nearly 85 percent of student institutional departures are voluntary. They A-n urn- occur despite the maintenance of adequate levels of academic performance" (Tinto, 1985, p.32). Research has consistently revealed that less than 25% of all institutional departures are due to poor academic performance (Astin, 1977; Tinto, 1993). Understanding Student Attrition Considerable research has attempted to uncover the reasons students leave institutions. Information is often obtained through exit interviews with students or post- departure surveys in which students indicate their reason(s) for leaving the institution by selecting the reason from a pre-determined list, or responding to short answer questions. Thus, students may be forced to choose the answer which most closely relates to their situation. While this information may be easily reported, it may not fully reflect the complexity of student departure. In a national study conducted in 1977, involving students from 358 institutions, dropouts most frequently cited boredom with courses, a change in career plans, financial difficulties, and dissatisfaction with requirements as the reasons for institutional departure (Astin, 1977). Academic boredom and uncertainty about academic plans, adjustment problems, unrealistic expectations about college, and financial difficulties continue to be themes among those students who leave (Noel, 1985; Upcraft & Gardner, 1989). These categories highlight the reasons for departure; yet provide little assistance in understanding why students leave college. Financial difficulties may be overstated by students as a reason for their departure. For some students, indicating financial problems may be more socially acceptable or may be simpler than trying to explain a more complex set of reasons. While many students may indeed have financial difficulties, money more often has to do with college access 14 and choice than with persistence (Noel, 1985). Indeed, many students incur significant debt in order to stay in school. One of the myths about attrition is that students who drop out of college actually flunk out; that is, they are asked to leave the institution due to unsatisfactory academic performance. Several studies have revealed that dropouts often have grade point averages equal to or greater than the grade point averages of persisting students (Noel, 1985). Certainly, for some students, poor academic performance weighs heavily in their own cost-benefit analysis of college attendance. A significant number of researchers have tried to predict and explain attrition. Various models have been suggested that emphasize different sets of variables and their relation to attrition. Variables have included background characteristics such as gender, ethnicity, social economic status, parents’ education, high school grade point average, and commitments to the institution and to the goal of college completion. Other predictive models have included measures of involvement and activities during the college experience such as place of residence, number of informal contacts with faculty, the nature of peer group interactions, and participation in extracurricular activities. Despite the inclusion of over 100 variables in models of attrition, tests of the models have only explained 10 to 20% of the variance in attrition (Astin, 1977; Panos & Astin, 1968; Terenzini & Pascarella, 1978; Tinto, 1975). In a national study involving students at 248 institutions, Panos and Astin (1968) investigated the relationship between 120 student background variables and persistence over four years of college. Their findings highlighted the complexity of accurately predicting student attrition. Of the 120 variables, only 20 entered into the final regression 15 equation, and accounted for only 9% of the variance in the group of students who had dropped out of college. They also found several environmental effects (e.g., interpersonal relationships and institutional policies) to be significantly related to persistence. Thus, completion of four years or more of college seems to be determined by the students’ personal characteristics and the environmental context of the institution (Panos & Astin, 1968). In a sample of 400 freshmen, variables related to students’ backgrounds and early college experiences were used to predict attrition. When only background variables were used in the prediction, six of the eight dropouts were predicted correctly. When the early college experience variables were included in the prediction model, however, all eight dropouts were predicted correctly (Nelson et al., 1984). Tinto (1993) proposed three categories of factors which may lead to early departure: individual, institutional, and external. The individual factors include the goals and intentions of the students upon entry into the institution. The greater the student’s commitment to the institution and to goals of obtaining a degree, the greater the persistence. Institutional factors include the experiences of students once enrolled. Issues of adjustment, degree of academic difficulty and congruence between the individual and the environment, and the amount of isolation experienced by students are all institutional factors. Finally, external factors which may contribute to departure include finances and obligations (e.g., personal and family) which pull the student away from the institution. 16 Attrition from Science and Engineering Majors Of specific interest for this study is research which documents the projected shortage of scientists and engineers over the next several years. Atkinson (1990) suggests that the problem is one of initial recruitment into science and engineering majors as well as students who drop out of college or who change from science and engineering majors to other fields. For the high school class of 1980, only 46% of the first year students declaring a major in science or engineering actually graduated with those degrees (Atkinson, 1990). For women and minority students, participation rates in science and engineering are not increasing, despite significant grth of those groups in the general population (Atkinson, 1990; Pool, 1990). The attrition rates for women from science and engineering are higher than those for men, even though women are not less well prepared academically, nor do they have lower grade point averages (Seymour, 1992). A theme identified in research by Seymour and Hewitt (Seymour, 1992) was that women in science and engineering encountered more difficulties in their educational experiences than their male peers did. Female students were more critical of the science and engineering teaching and reported it difficult to learn from faculty who took no personal interest in them, seemingly representing a desire for more affective orientation to teaching. Pool (1990) suggests that the underrepresentation of minority students in science and engineering is not an easily corrected one; the difficulties for minority group students begin much earlier than the college level. Atkinson (1990) suggests that the 17 nation's schools must provide a more supportive environment and one in which minority students are encouraged to pursue the sciences as fields of study. Many students who enter college declaring majors in science and engineering actually switch to majors outside of those fields. Seymour ( 1995) has investigated reasons students give for switching out of science and engineering majors. Findings from her research that are particularly meaningful in the context of the ROSES program include the following reasons given by students who switch out of these majors: inadequate high school preparation in study skills and basic courses; poor academic advising and lack of academic assistance; absence of peer study group support; and a competitive culture which leads to discouragement (Seymour, 1992). Tinto’s Model of Longitudinal Departure In contrast to identifying individual variables which lead to student departure, Tinto (1975) proposed that decisions to leave were made over time and were a result of the interactions between the student and the academic and social systems of the institution. Tinto recognized that students enter institutions with a variety of family background characteristics such as social status and parental education. Students also bring their own unique attributes and abilities (e.g., sex, ethnicity, and academic ability), as well as different expectations and motivations. Tinto suggested that the combination of these background attributes leads to a set of initial plans (intentions) and commitments to the institution and to the goal of college completion. He further suggested that interactions between the individual student and the academic and social systems continually modify the goal and institutional commitments in ways which lead to persistence or dropout. External factors, such as work or other commitments, are also 18 acknowledged as factors which may play a role in decisions to leave school (Tinto, 1975). The Tinto model focuses primarily on the nature of experiences and interactions that occur after students enter the institution rather than on pre-college characteristics. Tinto identified formal and informal components of the academic and social systems of the institution. The academic system includes those activities centered around formal education. The social system is comprised of interactions among students, faculty, and staff that occur outside of the formal academic setting. In both systems, there are formal (more structured) and informal (less structured) components. It is the nature of interactions in the academic and social systems, which influences the degree of academic integration and social integration which, in turn, contribute to revised intentions and commitments. Thus, positive experiences serve as integrative experiences and lead to strengthened intentions and commitments, while negative experiences are non-integrative and weaken the intentions and commitments. Over time, decisions to remain or leave the institution result from this process. Tinto’s model is presented in Figure 1. ’19 :2: .3 $225 ammo-EU we 3.58255 of. ”ems-EU doc-52. Even—Hm no 850 use 8550 2: mac—550m Homo—BU w5>a3 .33: 25... ”coed—om 93330 REES-$55 me 552 922.5. - 5 833m A B2: 589m .208 a; - 12525 828885 25:58:80 955 doom 35825800 35me L .8:me =23~w85 \ V 0 38m 8553. H mczoonom 5353821; I- I--- I is I \ .25 3:255:80 A I .-I EFF—om / 3:05-5:50 3:28:35 Ii I -1 3:23:55 use :80 F 3585 23 .25 . . . E2280 AI 0 c2828.: 28 £55 Bataan bmwmkzsomm H . coca-.325 _m=2E25_ 220332 4/ 82202: H r Ill. -. III-II III - I L i. III-...: II- IIIIIIIIII. IL / vascuwv—omm €52 583m 252390.. 3555800 c2535: moocotaxm 3:252:80 8.3.52 oEooSO can 280 o>smEoZ>m=oflcm 3:25:65 was 280 bEm-Em 20 Academic and Social lntggration Tinto’s theoretical contribution to the study of voluntary attrition is the centrality of academic and social integration and the longitudinal effects of interactions between individuals and the academic and social systems. "Given individual characteristics, prior experiences, and commitments, the model argues that it is the individual's integration into the academic and social systems of the college that most directly relates to his continuance in that college" (Tinto, 1975). Tinto does not suggest that full integration in both systems is necessary for persistence; however, some degree of membership in both the academic and social systems is important. Individual students may belong to several different systems or sub-communities and have different experiences in each of them. Much research has been conducted to test Tinto’s model of student departure. While different studies have focused on different aspects of the model, many of them have found support for the importance of academic and social integration (Nelson et al., 1984; Pascarella & Terenzini, 1980; Stoecker et al., 1988; Terenzini & Pascarella, 1976; Terenzini & Pascarella, 1977; Terenzini & Pascarella, 1978). Despite the different ways academic integration and social integration are operationally defined, Pascarella and Terenzini (1991) draw two general conclusions about academic and social integration from their summary of the research on this topic: First, at primarily residential institutions, social integration tends to have a compensatory interaction with academic integration and vice versa. A second generalization is that levels of either social or academic integration tend to have a compensatory influence on freshman-to—sophomore persistence for students who either enter a residential institution with characteristics predictive of withdrawal (for example, low family educational status, low educational aspirations) or who subsequently have low commitment to the institution or the goal of graduation from college. (pp. 411-412) 21 Transition to College The academic and social integration at the core of Tinto’s model of college student attrition is drawn from van Gennep’s (1960) model of life transitions. Van Gennep, an anthropologist, was especially interested in the ceremonies which often accompany an individual’s movement from one life position to another or from one social situation to another. Though the activities of the specific ceremonies varied depending on the ceremony, it was possible to identify three major phases in terms of the order and content of the ceremonial activities. Van Gennep (1960) labeled the phases separation, transition, and incorporation; together, he referred to the overall pattern as “rites of passage.” The rites of passage framework may be applied to the process by which a high school student becomes a college student (Tinto, 1988; 1993). Relative to student departure, the focus is on the early stages of the interactions between entering students and members of the institution. The separation phase involves separating from the past. More specifically, for students entering college, this includes a separation from the activities and habits of high school, and most likely includes some degree of separation from home, family, and friends. The transition phase is characterized by interactions with people (e.g., students, faculty, and staff) who are members of the college community. Finally, incorporation is the degree to which the student becomes a competent member of the new community. This requires learning the norms and establishing connections with others. While each stage has its defining characteristics, the stages may not be clearly distinguished from one another. Though there is considerable variability among 22 individual students as they pass through the stages, there are some commonalties. It is during this process that new students must learn the necessary skills to become a college student, despite their competence as a high school student. They may interact with different kinds of people, or in ways quite different from their previous interactions. Students may experience feelings of isolation, confusion, loneliness, and normlessness as they pass from one life and social position to another (Tinto, 1993). Zeller (1993) suggested the transition is analogous to the culture shock experienced by people moving into an unfamiliar culture, during which highpoints and lowpoints are experienced. He described new students as moving from the exciting days of orientation and welcoming activities to the shocking realities of academic work and meeting new people. Students find stability and comfort as they develop routines, but they experience doubts as their academic ability is challenged by first semester grades and the honeymoon period of new relationships wanes. As students begin to form relationships with other people and become involved in the university, they become integrated members of the environment. Adjustment to College Adjustment, as defined in this study, is the process of meeting the various demands in the collegiate setting in order to become academically and socially integrated in the university. The transition to university traditionally regarded as positive, involving new opportunities, nevertheless involves change for all students. There is the need to break with old routines and to adjust to the demands of a new environment including the need to adapt to the new intellectual and social challenges which present themselves. (Fisher & Hood, 1987, p. 425) 23 Draper (1991) suggested that adjustment takes time and requires students to try new behaviors, building on patterns of behavior that are successful. He also suggested that adjustment is “associative in the sense that these patterns are reinforced as freshmen interact with other students who share the same needs and goals” (p.73)- Successful adjustment significantly enhances the success and persistence of new students. For new students, there is transition and adjustment academically, socially, and personally. Students need to feel connected to other people in the institution; they need to have a sense of comfort with their academic and social environments; and they need assistance with issues related to their transition (Upcrafi & Gardner, 1989). Adjustment, then, is multifaceted, and is concerned with the “ability to cope effectively with the varying demands of the new college setting”(Baker & Siryk, 1989, p. 465). Adjustment during the first semester has been found to be a useful predictor of second semester persistence (Nelson et al., 1984). In fact, Krotseng (1992) found that scores on adjustment measures correctly classified 85 percent of all respondents as persisters or nonpersisters after one semester. Bragg (1994) found that student adjustment to college was highly correlated with student intent-to-persist, and was negatively correlated with consideration of institutional withdrawal. In summary, the adjustment process is the process through which students negotiate their transition from high school to college to become academically and socially integrated into the institution. Since the nature of interactions in the academic and social systems is critical to student persistence, research about adj ustment to college is valuable. 24 Research about Student Adjustment to College Adjustment to college has been the focus of two general categories of research studies. Relationships between a number of individual or background variables and various facets of adjustment to college have been examined. A smaller number of studies have treated adjustment as a dependent variable, investigating whether specific interventions or environmental characteristics influence adjustment to college. Correlates of Adjustment In a study of 56 first-year students, both individual and environmental variables were found to correlate with different aspects of adjustment. Problem-solving skills and ACT scores significantly predicted academic adjustment; while age, family income, perceived social support, and distance from home all related to positive social adjustment (Brooks II & DuBois, 1995 )- Mooney et al- (1991) surveyed 88 female college students to assess academic locus of control, self-esteem, and geographical distance from home as predictors of adjustment to college. They found that an internal locus of control, a high level of self- esteem, and the perception that the distance from home was “just right” (regardless of the actual distance) all contributed significantly to accurate predictions of college adjustment. It may be that distance from home measured by miles is less important than the feelings students have about being away from home. Feelings of homesickness are often reported by students, especially in the first few weeks of the semester. Fisher and Hood (1987) found that homesick students showed higher levels of psychological disturbance and lower levels of college adjustment than students who were not homesick. 25 Several studies have investigated family relationships and student adj ustment to college. Lapsley, Rice, and FitzGerald (1990) found that adolescents with strong attachment relationships with parents had a more positive adjustment to college than students who were insecurely attached. In another study, psychological separation was unrelated to college adjustment in men and negatively correlated with adjustment in women (Lopez, Campbell, & Watkins, 1986). There is some evidence of variation in the relationship between attachment with parents and adjustment to college depending on the class standing of students. Lapsley et al. (1990) reported that for freshmen, parental attachment variables accounted for a significant amount of variation only in academic adjustment, while for juniors, parental attachment variables accounted for significant amounts of variation in academic, social, and personal adjustment as well as goal commitment. In another study, Lapsley, Rice, and Shadid (1989) found that freshmen were more psychologically dependent on their parents and had lower levels of social and personal adjustment to college than upperclass students. The nature of family relationships may also play a role in student adjustment to college. Lopez et al. (1988) found a relationship between conflicted parent-student attachments and college adjustment for both men and women. While this relationship may have been affected by variables not controlled for, it still suggested that a conflicted relationship with parents may create emotional difficulties for the students which, in turn, may impact aspects of college adjustment. In Lopez’s (1991) study, women and students who reported high levels of marital conflict in their families displayed lower levels of personal adjustment to college- 26 In three separate studies, Smith and Baker (1987) found a positive relationship between freshman decidedness regarding academic major and adj ustment to college. In all three studies, students who were more certain about their choice of academic major, as compared to those who were less certain, scored higher in at least one area of adjustment. There was some evidence that this relationship was more evident in the second rather than the first semester. There is also evidence that adjustment to college is likely to be related to factors not only within the person, but also in the institutional environment (Barthelemy & Fine, 1995; Brooks II & DuBois, 1995; Cooper & Robinson, 1988). This is consistent with Tinto’s view that what happens after a student arrives on campus is critical. Freshmen who report a high degree of alienation are less likely to be involved in campus activities, are more likely to be dissatisfied with their college experience, and are more likely to experience adjustment difficulties. Alienated students are also more likely to discontinue their enrollment, either for a short time or permanently (Baker & Siryk, 1980). Social effectiveness and social adjustment seem to be significantly related to overall adjustment to college and to persistence (Baker & Siryk, 1983; Christie & Dinham, 1991). While Harris (1991) found social and academic adj ustment to predict goal and institutional attachment, social adjustment explained more variance in attachment (30%) than academic adjustment (6%). In The Transition to College Project (Terenzini et al., 1992), focus groups with diverse groups of students from four distinctly different institutions revealed the importance of social and intellectual validation. In some ways, perhaps no theme was more persistent throughout the interviews—regardless of race or ethnicity, gender, age, or institution 27 attended—than new students’ need for self-esteem in its many forms: self-confidence, a sense of being in control, pride in oneself and what one does, respecting oneself and being respected by others, valuing oneself and being valued by others. (Terenzini et al., 1996) The validation may come from parents, peers, faculty, or staff and can take the form of simple words of encouragement. For students of color attending primarily white, residential institutions, social validation and adjustment seem particularly important (Murguia et al., 1991; Stoecker et al., 1988; Terenzini et al., 1996). Astin (1977) suggested that environmental circumstances could significantly increase the chance of students completing college. Living in residence halls during the first year of college has a positive effect on adjustment. In a study by Wilson, Anderson, and Fleming (1987), first year students who commuted from home demonstrated poorer personal adjustment than freshmen who lived in residence halls. Bragg (1994) also found that commuters, compared to students who lived on campus, did not make as many friends, were less active, did not adjust to college as well, and gave more consideration to dropping out. Blimling (1993), in a review of research about residential living, suggested that the nature of the social environment in a residence hall encourages greater personal development of students than living at home. “Immersion in the college experience requires students to adapt to the demands of a new environment and, in doing so, forces them to alter perceptions and to learn new ways of interacting in that environment " (Blimling, 1993, p. 263). At the same time, residential students find comfort in the fact that others around them are also in transition, thus, transition is viewed as a c00perative endeavor (Johnson, Staton, & Jorgensen—Earp, 1995; Terenzini et al., 1992). 28 In an investigation of the effect of residence hall climate on college adjustment, Barthelemy and Fine (1995) found hall climate variables of personal support and group cohesiveness positively correlated with adjustment, while a climate of conflict was negatively related to adjustment. Although this study was correlational in nature, Barthelemy suggested that residence hall climate variables do play a role in student adjustment to college, and could be manipulated to maximize adjustment. Effects of Interventions on Adjustment There are some studies which treat student adjustment to college as a dependent variable. All students experience transition from high school and home to college. The identification of strategies that could influence positive adjustment would be very helpful to students and institutions. In Baker’s (1986) investigation of the effect of an intervention on adjustment, students who scored below the means on four subscales of an adj ustment survey were divided into control and experimental groups. Students in the experimental group were invited to an individual interview to discuss their scores. Students who participated in the interview, compared with students in the control group, showed significantly greater improvement in adjustment scores on the post-test survey. Additionally, significantly fewer of the interviewed students withdrew from college. The researchers realized that the observed effect may have had little to do with the content of the conversation. It could have been that the retention effect was instead due to a feeling by the interviewed students that someone at the institution cared about them. The literature reports mixed results regarding the effects of summer orientation programs on adjustment. Orientation programs are generally designed to assist new 29 students in their transition to college. Pascarella et a1. (1986) found that students who attended an orientation program had significantly higher levels of social integration and subsequent commitment to the institution than those who did not attend orientation. Orientation attendance, however, had no effect on adjustment to college or on student persistence in a study by Martin and Dixon (1989). In both cases, orientation attendance was voluntary, which makes it more difficult to eliminate any pre-orientation intentions or motivations which might explain the findings. Schwitzer, McGovern, and Robbins (1991) investigated the effects of a freshman orientation course and found that participation in the course was associated with improved academic and social adj ustment. Participation in the course was voluntary and there was no comparison group of freshmen, so the actual effect of the course is unclear. High school students who enrolled in a college orientation course (University 101) during high school anticipated being able to adjust to college more successfully than a group of high school students who were enrolled in a college government course (Buchanan, 1991). Data from this study indicated that students in the University 101 group had greater actual adjustment to college than students in either the group taking the government course or students in a control group taking no college course; however, the finding was not statistically significant. Research comparing the experiences of students in residence halls to those who live in living-learning centers suggests that student adjustment is enhanced by the living- learning environment. Pemberton (1969) found no significant differences after one year of college between the academic performance of freshmen in a living—learning center and those in conventional halls. However, students in the living-learning center believed their 30 transition to college was made easier due to the fiiendly, cohesive, and supportive atmosphere of the living-learning center. Surveying seniors at Michigan State University, Nosow (1975) found that seniors in three living-learning centers were more positive in their attitudes about their personal adjustment, well-being, and intellectual growth than seniors in conventional residence halls. Draper (1991) spent a year in a predominately freshman residence hall during the 1978-1979 academic year. His ethnographic work focused on nine first-year students and their transition to the university. As a result of his study, he makes the following recommendations about on-campus housing for first-year students. Freshmen should be housed in proximity to one another for purposes of support and group identity, yet they should not be isolated from upper division students or faculty or staff of the institution. In fact, he suggested that academic and student affairs offices should be located in residence halls to provide casual contact with students. He encouraged the development of strategies to overcome the real and perceived barriers between new students and the academic life of the institution. Finally, he suggested that faculty and staff should have opportunities to interact informally with students, and that the number of co-curricular activities in the halls should be increased. Residential Option for Science and Engineering Students (ROSES) The ROSES program for science and engineering students incorporates many of the recommendations made by Draper and addresses some of the factors cited by students who switch out of science and engineering majors. The purpose of the ROSES program, as described in the pilot program proposal, is “. . . to actively pursue the improvement in the quality of the undergraduate experience for those students who choose science and engineering as 31 academic and professional careers where academic subject matter is both common and challenging as well as to provide a sense of belongingness and community where too often the university environment and academic rigor come into conflict for undergraduates.” (E. M. Wilson, personal communication, Spring, 1993) F irst-year students who have expressed interest in science and engineering majors may elect to participate in the ROSES program. Students in the program are housed together in the same residence hall, and most are assigned roommates who are also in the ROSES program. All students in the ROSES program are required to enroll in a ROSES seminar class during the fall semester. The seminar is a one-credit course and is designed to assist students in their transition to college by exposing them to resources at the institution, helping them learn academic skills, and introducing them to their field of study. The class meets once per week in a classroom and students are expected, as part of the class, to attend co-curricular activities planned by the residence life staff Each participating college is responsible for the seminar class for their students. The specific topics covered in the class may vary somewhat among the three colleges. Resident assistants (undergraduate staff members) receive some additional training related to the ROSES program so they can be of assistance to new students. They are also involved in the implementation of the co-curricular portions of the ROSES seminar class. Several returning students serve as peer mentors and tutors for new students. Faculty and academic advisers serve as instructors for the ROSES seminar class and are encouraged to interact with students in the residence hall, via advising appointments or informal conversations. Special tutoring rooms are also available in the residence hall for group study and for tutoring at designated times. 32 Summary In this chapter, the transition from high school to college has been examined in the context of students’ academic and social integration into the institution. Students’ experiences with the academic and social systems of the institution may be instrumental in helping them adjust to college. There is evidence that the first year of college, especially the first six weeks, are key in shaping the remainder of the undergraduate experience. While a number of studies document relationships between background variables and adjustment to college, fewer studies have examined strategies which might positively affect adjustment. Several studies have suggested that the peer environment and social climate in residence halls are related to positive adjustment to college. This study sought to determine which components of a residential program, structured to integrate academic and social elements, significantly predicted student adjustment to college. A schematic representation of this study is presented in Figure 2. zen Es .Eogméé =sm u:uE%:€< 35cm J:o§§€< Essences :o 88.on meats»: m0< HEOm mQu< BEonno< 228885 332$ Empmwm UHEmQ .05). Table 3 presents the mean predicted grade point averages for each racial/ethnic group, as well as the results of the ANOVA. Due to the small number of respondents enrolled in the College of Agriculture and Natural 47 Resources, their responses were combined with responses from the College of Natural Science to yield two categories for college of enrollment: Engineering and non- Engineering. Table 3 —— Analysis of Variance of Predicted Grade Point Average by Minority Group Racial/Ethnic group Predicted gpg_ African American (11 = 11) 2.61 Hispanic (n = 3) 2.81 Native American Q1 = 3) 2.57 Asian American (11 = 9) 2.84 Other (11 = 1) 3.22 Total (n = 27) 2.73 F = l.787;p> .05 Adjustment Scores for Respondents All respondents completed the Student Adaptation to College Questionnaire (Baker & Siryk, 1989), which yields scores for academic adjustment, social adjustment, attachment, personal-emotional adjustment, and full adjustment. For this study, scores from the academic adjustment and social adj ustment subscales, and the full score were used. Table 4 presents descriptive statistics for the adjustment scores for this sample. The descriptive statistics (i.e. means, standard deviations, and ranges) for the two subscales and the full score for this sample are similar to those obtained for samples from a number of colleges and universities throughout the United States (see Baker & Siryk, 1989). The scores for all of the adj ustment scores are normally distributed, as indicated by measures of skewness and kurtosis. Skewness and kurtosis values between +/- 1.0 are considered excellent for psychometric purposes (George and Mallery, 1995). 43 Table 4 - Descriptive Statistics for Adjustment Scores Adjustment Score n Range Mean Std. Skewness Kurtosis Deviation Academic 174 77-201 140.14 23.21 .079 .259 Social 174 71-174 132.95 21.71 -.516 .010 Full 174 254-568 418.49 60.98 .009 -.421 Table 5 presents the intercorrelations among the two subscales and full adjustment score. The intercorrelations are similar to those obtained in other administrations of the SACQ (see Baker & Siryk, 1989). Baker and Siryk (1989) assert that the “. .. correlations are large enough to indicate that the subscales are indeed measuring a common construct, but small enough to support the conceptualization of that construct as having different facets as represented by the subscales” (p.34). Table 5 - Correlations Among Academic, Social, and Full Adjustment Academic Social Full Academic 1 .446" .826" Social .446” 1 .806” Full .826" .806" 1 Note. it = 174; **g< .01 Adjustment Scores and Background Variables Descriptive and inferential statistics are presented for the adjustment scores and the background variables of gender, ethnicity, college of enrollment, and predicted grade point average. Gender Table 6 indicates the mean adj ustment scores for males and females, and provides the results of two—tailed t-tests for each adjustment score and gender. The t-tests revealed that females scored significantly higher on the academic adjustment subscale (t = 2.31; 49 that females scored significantly higher on the academic adjustment subscale (t = 2.31; p_< .05). There were no gender effects for the social adjustment subscale or the full SCOIC. Table 6 - T-tests of Means of Adjustment Scores by Gender Adjustment Males Females Score n= 110 n=64 x sd x sd t prob. Academic 137.02 22.21 145.52 24.07 2.31 .02* Social 131.70 20.43 135.11 23.76 .96 .34 Full 411.78 59.53 430.03 62.18 1.90 .06 *p_<.05 Ethnicity Table 7 presents the mean adj ustment scores for non-minority and minority students, as well as the results of two-tailed t-tests. There were no significant differences in adjustment scores between non-minority and minority participants in the ROSES program. Table 7 - T-tests of Means of Adjustment Scores by Ethnicity Adjustment Scores Non-minority Minority n = 147 n= 27 x sd x sd 1 prob. Academic 140.60 22.60 137.67 26.62 .538 .59 Social 132.15 21.85 137.33 20.78 -1.182 .25 Full 418.73 59.92 417.33 67.67 .108 .92 College of Enrollment Table 8 presents the means for adj ustment scores for students enrolled in the College of Engineering and those who were not in the College of Engineering, as well as 50 the results of two-tailed t-tests. There were no significant differences in the adjustment scores between students in the College of Engineering and those who were not in the College of Engineering. Table 8 - T-tests of Means of Adjustment Scores by College Adjustrnent Engineering Non-Engineering Scores n = 132 n = 42 x sd x sd t prob. Academic 140.07 23.86 140.38 21.32 -.O80 .94 Social 132.92 21.18 133.07 23.56 -.038 .97 Full 418.09 62.44 419.76 56.85 -.162 .87 Predicted Grade Point Average Predicted grade point average is based upon high school grade point average, test scores (ACT or SAT), and the quality of the high school attended. The median predicted grade point average for all respondents was 2.81, which was used to divide the respondents into two groups. Table 9 presents the means for adjustment scores for students with a predicted grade point average of 2.81 or higher and those with a predicted grade point average below a 2.81, as well as the results of two-tailed t-tests. Students in the higher predicted GPA group scored significantly higher on the academic adjustment subscale than students in the lower predicted GPA group (t = 2.276; p_< .05). There were no predicted grade point average effects on the social adjustment subscale or full score. 51 Table 9 - T-tests of Means of Adjustment Scores by Predicted Grade Point Average Adjustment Scores Predicted GPA Predicted GPA 22.81 <2.81 n=85 n=87 x sd x sd t prob. Academic 144.29 25.72 136.30 20.06 2.276 .02“ Social 133.86 21.72 131.66 21.72 .664 .51 Full 426.47 67.19 410.52 54.19 1.716 .09 *p_<.05 Summagy Academic adjustment scores were significantly higher for females (t = 2.31; p_< .05) and for respondents with predicted GPAs of 2.81 or higher (t = 2.276; p_< .05). There were no significant effects for any of the other background variables. Fall Semester Grade Point Averages for Respondents Fall semester grade point averages for all respondents ranged from .38 to 4.0, with the mean for the group being 2.86. This was very close to the 2.81 predicted grade point average for the entire group. Fall Semester Grade Point Average and Background Variables Descriptive and inferential statistics are presented for fall semester grade point average and the background variables of gender, ethnicity, college of enrollment, and predicted grade point average. Gender The fall semester grade point average for females was 3.03, which was significantly higher than the 2.76 fall grade point average for males (t = 2.34; p_< .05), 52 even though there were no significant differences between the predicted grade point averages for males and females (see Table 10). Table 10 - T-test of Means for Grade Point Averages by Gender Females Males n = 64 n = 110 x sa' x sd t prob. Predicted Grade Point Average 2.81 .257 2.80 .317 .33 .74 Fall Semester Grade Point Average .03 .688 2.76 .797 2.34 .02“ *g< .05 Ethnicity Non-minority students had a fall semester grade point average of 2.93, which was significantly higher than the 2.51 received by minority students (t = .64; p_< .05). There was no significant difference in the predicted grade point average between minority and non—minority respondents (see Table 11). Table 11 - T-tests of Means for Grade Point Averages by Ethnicity Non-minority Minority n = 147 n = 27 x sd x sd t prob. Predicted Grade Point Average 2.82 .293 2.73 .302 1.391 .17 Fall Semester Grade Point Average 2.93 .675 2.51 1.103 2.63 .01“ *p_< .05 53 College of Enrollment A two-tailed t-test revealed no significant difference in the fall semester grade point averages between students enrolled in Engineering and students not in Engineering, even though the predicted grade point average for students in Engineering was significantly higher than students not in Engineering (t = 2.034; p_< .05). The means and results of the t-tests are presented in Table 12. Table 12 - T-Tests for Fall Grade Point Averages by College Non- Engineering Engineering n = 132 n = 42 x sd x sd t Jrob. Fall Semester Grade Point Average 2.87 .803 2.84 .654 .192 .85 Predicted Grade Point Average 2.83 .293 2.72 .293 2.034 .05* *p_= .05 Predicted Grade Point Average Students with higher predicted grade point averages earned a fall semester grade point average of 3. 14, which was significantly higher than the 2.61 fall grade point average earned by respondents who had lower predicted grade point averages (t = 4.798; g< .01). Table 13 presents the mean grade point averages and the results of a two-tailed t-test. 54 Table 13 - T-Test for Fall Semester Grade Point Average by Predicted GPA Predicted GPA Predicted GPA 2 2.81 < 2.81 n = 85 n = 87 x sd x sd 1 prob. Fall Semester GPA 3.14 .669 2.61 .769 4.798 .00** **p_< .01 Summagy Fall semester grade point averages were significantly higher for females (t = 2.34; p_< .05), non-minority respondents (t = 2.64; p_< .01), and for respondents with higher predicted grade point averages (t = 4.798; p_< .01). ROSES Experiences Survey Data for Respondents All respondents completed a ROSES Experiences Survey, which was designed for this study. The purpose of the survey was to ascertain the levels of involvement by the respondents in the various components of the ROSES program. Some of these components are unique to the ROSES program and are not available to other students at the university. For purposes of this study, these components are considered directly related to the ROSES program. There are other activities that most students are engaged in, such as spending time in class. Though they are only indirectly related to the ROSES program, the programmatic design and emphasis of the ROSES program suggested that these components also be explored for their contribution to adjustment and grade point average. The components were grouped into two categories: academic and social. The academic components included the directly related components of students’ satisfaction with the contact with their academic adviser, how well students felt their 55 ROSES seminar instructor knew them, and the degree to which students felt the ROSES program had helped them academically. These components were scored on a Likert scale. The academic components indirectly related to the ROSES program included the number of hours spent in academic activities (i.e. class, studying). The social components directly related to the ROSES program included whether or not the roommate was in the ROSES program; the proportion of time students spent studying with other ROSES students; and the degree to which they felt the ROSES program had helped them socially. The indirect social components included whether students knew their roommate prior to attending MSU; their satisfaction with their roommate relationship and with the contact with a resident assistant; and the number of hours students spent in non-instructional activities (i.e. work, student groups, community service). Satisfaction items were scored on a Likert scale. The final component is the ROSES seminar, a unique component required of all ROSES students. The seminar grade was based on an accumulation of points students earned for attending class, attending evening seminars in the residence hall, and completing in-class and out-of-class assignments. The grade received in the seminar serves as a reasonable proxy of the degree to which students were involved in the seminar. Table 14 presents descriptive statistics for the academic and social components of the ROSES program, as well as the ROSES seminar grade. 56 Table 14 - Descriptive Statistics for ROSES Components ROSES Component N Range Mean Std. Deviation ACADEMIC Class (hours/week) 172 0-30 14.9 4.0 Study (hours/week) 173 1-45 15.2 8.28 Satisfied with adviser 173 0-5* 3.1 1.53 Known by ROSES instructor 173 1-5** 2.7 1.02 ROSES helped academically 173 1-5** 3.0 1.21 SOCIAL Involvement (hours/week) 174 0-41 8.8 7.64 Study with ROSES (proportiog 172 00-75 .17 .17 Satisfied with roommate 172 0—5* 3.7 1.39 Satisfied with RA 173 0-5* 3.9 1.28 ROSES helped socially 173 1-5** 3.4 1.31 ROSES seminar grade (4.0 scale) 174 0.0-4.0 3.6 .81 Note. * scored on a Likert scale and recoded with O = does not apply; 5 = very satisfied ** scored on a Likert scale with 1 = not at all; 5 = very well ROSES Exgriences Survey and Background Variables Descriptive and inferential statistics are presented for the measures associated with the ROSES components and the background variables of gender, ethnicity, college of enrollment, and predicted grade point average. Gender Table 15 presents the mean responses for each of the ROSES components for males and females, and provides the results of two-tailed t-tests for each item and gender. The t-tests revealed that females were significantly more satisfied with their contact with an academic adviser (t = 2.28; p_< .05). Females were also more positive than males in their feeling that the ROSES program had helped them academically (t = 4.20; p_< .01) and socially (t = 4.89; p_< .01). 57 Table 15 - Means of ROSES Components by Gender ROSES Component Males Females n=110 n=64 x sd x sd I prob. ACADEMIC Class (hours/week) 15.0 3.68 14.7 4.51 .502 .62 Study (hours/week) 14.6 8.86 16.1 7.15 1.238 .22 Satisfied with adviser 2.9 1.54 3.5 1.46 2.276 .02* Known by ROSES instructor 2.7 1.04 2.8 .96 .706 .48 ROSES helped academically 2.7 1.26 3.5 .96 4.201 .00" SOCIAL Involvement (hours/week) 9.1 7.65 8.2 7.67 .799 .43 Study with ROSES (proportion) .18 .17 .16 .18 .700 .49 Satisfied with roommate 3.6 1.29 3.8 1.55 .722 .47 Satisfied with RA 3.9 1.29 3.8 1.28 .383 .70 ROSES helped socially 3.1 1.34 4.0 1.01 4.888 .00" ROSES seminar grade 3.6 .88 3.8 .64 1.769 .08 *p_< .05; **p_< .01 Ethnicity Table 16 presents the mean responses for the ROSES components for non- minority and minority reSpondents, as well as the results of two-tailed t-tests. The only significant difference between non-minority and minority students was the ROSES seminar grade (t = 3.44; p_< .01) with non-minority students earning significantly higher grades in the seminar than minority students. 58 Table 16 - Means of ROSES Components by Ethnicity ROSES Component Non-minority Minority n = 147 n = 27 x sd x sd 1 prob. ACADEMIC Class (hours/week) 14.9 3.89 14.8 4.64 .175 .86 Study (hours/week) 15.2 8.11 15.1 9.32 .042 .97 Satisfied with adviser 3.1 1.50 3.2 1.69 .178 .86 Known by ROSES instructor 2.7 1.01 2.8 1.09 .352 .73 ROSES helped academically 3.0 1.21 3.2 1.21 .784 .43 SOCIAL Involvement (hours/week) 8.5 7.67 10.5 7.43 1.299 .20 Study with ROSES (proportion) .16 .17 .23 .19 1.557 .13 Satisfied with roommate 3.7 1.40 3.7 1.41 .001 .99 Satisfied with RA 3.9 1.25 3.7 1.44 .632 .53 ROSES helped socially 3.4 1.30 3.6 1.37 .784 .43 ROSES seminar grade 3.7 .61 3.2 1.38 3.437 .00* *p_< .01 College of Enrollment The means for the ROSES components for students enrolled in the College of Engineering and those not enrolled in Engineering are presented in Table 17, as well as the results of two-tailed t-tests. The t-tests revealed that students enrolled in the College of Engineering were significantly less satisfied with their contact with an academic adviser (t = 3.405; g< .01) than were non-engineering students. The non-engineering students were also more positive in their feeling that the ROSES program had helped them academically (t = 5.224; p_< .01); and socially (t = 3.251; p_< .01). 59 Table 17 - Means of ROSES Components by College ROSES Component Engineering Non-Engineering n = 132 n = 42 - x sd x sd t prob. ACADEMIC Class (hours/week) 14.9 3.96 14.8 4.16 .149 .88 Study (hours/weelg 14.7 8.19 16.7 8.46 1.383 .17 Satisfied with adviser 2.9 1.56 3.8 1.17 3.405 .00" Known by ROSES instructor 2.6 1.00 2.9 1.05 1.566 .12 ROSES helped academically 2.8 1.19 3.8 .92 5.224 .00" SOCIAL Involvement (hours/week) 8.5 6.96 9.8 9.51 .828 .41 Study with ROSES (proportig) .17 .16 .19 .20 .648 .52 Satisfied with roommate 3.7 1.33 3.7 1.60 .098 .92 Satisfied with RA 3.8 1.34 4.0 1.07 .944 .35 ROSES helped socially 3.2 1.34 3.9 1.08 3.251 .00“ ROSES seminar grade 3.6 .83 3.6 .72 .020 .98‘ **p_< .0] Predicted Grade Point Average The median of all predicted grade point averages (2.81) was used to divide the respondents into two groups: those with predicted grade point averages greater than or equal to 2.81 and those with predicted grade point averages below 2.81. The means for the ROSES components for respondents in the two categories are presented in Table 18, as well as the results of two-tailed t-tests. The t-tests revealed that respondents with a predicted grade point average of 2.81 or higher felt significantly more well known by their ROSES instructor (t = 2.38; g< .05); reported spending significantly more hours per week in class (t = 1.987; p_= .05), and earned a significantly higher grade in the ROSES seminar class (t = 2.616; p_< .05). 6O Table 18 - Means of ROSES Components by Predicted GPA ROSES Component Predicted GPA Predicted GPA 2 2.81 < 2.81 n = 84 n = 87 x sd x Sd t prob. ACADEMIC Class (hours/week) 15.54 4.45 14.32 3.46 1.987 .05“ Study (hours/week) 16.36 8.63 13.98 7.88 1.881 .06 Satisfied with adviser 3.30 1.45 2.94 1.58 1.530 .13 Known by ROSES instructor 2.89 1.01 2.53 1.00 2.38 .02M ROSES helped academically 3.06 1.28 2.98 1.15 .442 .66 SOCIAL Involvement (hours/week) 9.56 7.91 7.71 6.93 1.632 .10 Study with ROSES (proportion) .15 .14 .19 .20 1.519 .13 Satisfied with roommate 3.81 1.43 3.59 1.38 1.007 .32 Satisfied with RA 3.93 1.34 3.77 1.23 .805 .42 ROSES helped socially 3.48 1.27 3.35 1.31 .645 .52 ROSES seminargrade 3.81 .58 3.49 .94 2.616 .01“ *p_= .05; **p_< .05 Summag Comparisons of mean differences on gender, ethnicity, predicted GPA, and college of enrollment showed significant differences in the degree of participation in the ROSES program. Females (t = 2.276; p< .05) and non-Engineering students (t = 3.405; g< .01) were significantly more satisfied with the contact with an academic adviser. Females (t = 4.201; p_< .01) and non-Engineering students (t = 5.224; p_< .01) were also significantly more positive about the degree to which they felt the ROSES program had helped them academically. Females (t = 4.888; g< .01) and non-Engineering students (t = 3.251; p_< .01) were significantly more positive about the degree to which they felt the ROSES program had helped them socially as well. Students with a predicted grade point average of 2.81 or higher felt known by their ROSES instructor to a significantly higher degree than those students with a predicted grade point average below a 2.81 61 (t = 2.38; g< .05). Finally, minority students (t = 3.437; p_< .01) and students with a predicted grade point average below a 2.81 (t = 2.616; p_< .05) received significantly lower grades in the ROSES seminar class. Research Questions and Hypotheses Two research questions guided this study: 1) Do individual components of the ROSES program contribute differentially to academic adj ustment, social adjustment, full adjustment, and fall semester grade point average; and 2) Which components of the ROSES program make the most significant contributions to predicting academic adjustment, social adjustment, full adjustment, and fall semester grade point average. Research hypotheses were stated in Chapter 3. In this chapter, they are formulated as statistical hypotheses stated in the null form to test relationships between individual components of the ROSES program and adjustment and fall semester grade point average. Results The relationships between individual components of the ROSES program and two sets of outcomes-- adjustment scores (academic, social, and full) and fall semester grade point average-- were explored. Bivariate analyses between individual variables and outcomes provided a general overview of these relationships. To test relationships between individual components and outcomes, respondents were split into two groups at the median: those who had a greater degree of participation with the component and those who had a lesser degree of participation with the component. Multiple regression was used to identify significant determinants of adjustment and of fall semester grade point average. 62 Academic Adjustment The academic components directly related to the ROSES program included satisfaction with the contact with an academic adviser, the degree to which students felt known by their ROSES seminar instructor, and the degree to which students felt the ROSES program had helped them academically (i.e. classes, studying). Indirect academic components included the number of hours per week students reported being in class and studying. Table 19 presents the means for academic adjustment for the two groups, as well as the results of t-tests. Hymthesis 1: There is no difference in academic adjustment scores, as measured by the academic adjustment subscale of the SACQ, between students with a greater degree of participation in any of the academic components directly related to the ROSES program and those with a lesser degree of participation. _Sa_ti§f_action with aLdviser: A two-tailed t-test revealed no significant difference in academic adjustment scores between those who were more satisfied and those who were less satisfied with the contact with an academic adviser (t = 1.177; p_> .05). Feeligg_known by the ROSES instructor: There was no significant difference in academic adjustment scores for students who felt known to a greater degree by their ROSES instructor and those who felt known to a lesser degree (t = 1.692; p> .05). Feelinthat the ROSES program helped academically: There was no significant difference in academic adjustment scores between students who were most positive and those who were less positive in their feeling that the ROSES program had helped them academically (t = 1.859; p_> .05). 63 Hypothesis 1 is accepted. None of the academic components that were directly related to the ROSES program were significantly related to academic adjustment scores. Hypothesis 2: There is no difference in academic adjustment scores, as measured by the academic adjustment subscale of the SACQ, between students with a greater degree of participation in any of the academic components indirectly related to the ROSES program and those with a lesser degree of participation. _I-_I_Qt_r_rs per week in cla__s_s_: Students who reported being in class 15 hours per week or more achieved significantly higher academic adjustment scores than students who were in class for less than 15 hours per week (t = 2.044; g< .05). flggrfler week studying: Students who reported studying 15 hours per week or more achieved significantly higher academic adj ustment scores than students who studied less than 15 hours per week (t = 2.582; g< .05). Hypothesis 2 is rejected. The academic components indirectly related to the ROSES program were significantly related to academic adjustment. Students who spent more hours per week in class achieved significantly higher academic adjustment (t = 2.044; g< .05), as did students who spent more hours per week studying (t = 2.582; p_< .05). 64 Table 19-T-tests for Participation with Academic Components and Academic Adjustment ROSES Academic Component Adjustment x sd 1 prob. Satisfied with adviser 2 3 141.27 22.68 <3 136.07 24.99 1.177 .24 Known by ROSES instructor 2 3 142.66 23.40 < 3 136.68 22.79 1.692 .09 ROSES helped academically 2 3 142.33 22.79 < 3 135.22 23.72 1.859 .07 Hoursperweekinclass 2.15 142.78 24.19 <15 135.54 21.08 2.944 04* Hours per week studying Z 15 144.34 22.48 < 15 135.29 23.36 2.582 .01* *p_< .05 The social components unique to the ROSES program included having a ROSES roommate, studying with other ROSES students, satisfaction with the contact with a resident assistant, and feeling that the ROSES program had helped socially (i.e. meeting people, getting involved). The social components indirectly related to the ROSES program included knowing one’s roommate prior to attending MSU, satisfaction with the roommate relationship, and the number of hours per week students were involved in non- instructional activities (i.e. student groups, work, community service). Table 20 presents the means for academic adjustment as well as the results of t-tests for participation with the social components of the ROSES program and academic adjustment. Hypgthesis 3: There is no difference in academic adjustment scores, as measured by the academic adjustment subscale of the SACQ, between students with a greater degree of participation in any of the social components directly related to the ROSES program and those with a lesser degree of participation. 65 ROSES roommat_e_: There was no significant difference in the academic adjustment scores between students who had a roommate who was also in the ROSES program and those whose roommate was not in the ROSES program (t = 1.303; p > .05). Studyingwith ROSES students: There was no significant different in academic adjustment scores between students who studied more with other ROSES students and those who studied less with other ROSES students (t = -.866; p> .05). _S_agis_faction with resident agsistant: There was no significant difference between students who were more satisfied with the contact with a resident assistant and those who were less satisfied (t = .310; 9 > .05). Feeling that the ROSES progrim helped socim There was no significant difference in academic adjustment scores between students who were more positive that the ROSES program had helped them socially and those who were less positive (t = .823; p > .05). Hypothesis 3 is accepted. None of the social components directly related to the ROSES program were significantly related to academic adjustment. Hymthesis 4: There is no difference in academic adjustment scores, as measured by the academic adjustment subscale of the SACQ, between students with a greater degree of participation in any of the social components indirectly related to the ROSES program and those with a lesser degree of participation. 66 Prior knowledge of roomma_t_e_: Students who knew their roommate before attending MSU scored an average of 149.07 on the academic adjustment subscale which was significantly higher than the 137.42 scored by those who did not know their roommate prior to attendance (t = -2.214; p < .05). Satisfaction with roommate relationshg): There was no significant difference in academic adjustment scores between students who were more satisfied with their roommate relationship and those who were less satisfied (t = -.746; p > .05). Involvement: There was no significant difference in academic adjustment scores between students who were involved for three hours per week or more in non-instructional activities and those who were involved for less than three hour per week (t = -.069; p > .05). Hypothesis 4 is rejected. Prior knowledge of one’s roommate was significantly related to academic adjustment (t = -2.214; p < .05). No other indirect social component of the ROSES program was related to academic adjustment. 67 Table 20 - T-tests for Participation with Social Components and Academic Adjustment ROSES Academic Component Adjustment x s.d. t prob. ROSES roommate Yes 138.28 22.61 No 144.76 25.12 1.303 .20 Proportion study with ROSES 2 .13 138.74 23.39 < .13 141.83 23.30 -.866 .39 Satisfaction with RA 2 4 140.41 23.00 <4 139.15 24.18 .310 .76 ROSES helped socially 2 4 141.45 23.44 <4 138.54 23.10 .823 .41 Prior knowledge of roommate Yes 149.07 26.49 No 137.42 21.97 -2.214 .03"' Satisfaction with roommate 2 4 139.15 24.23 <4 141.75 21.36 -.716 .48 Hours per week involved 2 3 140.03 23.80 < 3 140.28 22.65 -.O69 .95 *p<.05 Social Adjustment The social components unique to the ROSES program included having a ROSES roommate, studying with other ROSES students, satisfaction with the contact with a resident assistant, and feeling that the ROSES program had helped socially (i.e. meeting people, getting involved). The social components indirectly related to the ROSES program included knowing one’s roommate prior to attending MSU, satisfaction with the roommate relationship, and the number of hours per week students were involved in non- instructional activities (i.e. student group, work, community service). Two-tailed t-tests were conducted to test for differences in social adjustment scores. For continuous variables, respondents were split into two groups at the median: those who had a greater degree of participation with the component and those who had a 68 lesser degree of participation with the component. Table 21 presents the means for social adjustment for the two groups, as well as the results of two-tailed t-tests. Hymthesis 5: There is no difference in social adjustment scores, as measured by the social adjustment su bscale of the SACQ, between students with a greater degree of participation in the social components directly related to the ROSES program and those with a lesser degree of participation. ROSES roommat_e_: There was no significant difference in social adjustment scores between students who had roommates who were also participating in the ROSES program and those whose roommates were not in the ROSES program (t = .402; p_> .05). Studfimwith ROSES students: A t-test revealed no significant difference in social adjustment between students who studied more with other ROSES students, and those who studied less with other ROSES students (t = .597; p_> .05). Sat_i§faction with resideflassistaaa Students who were more satisfied with the contact with a resident assistant scored an average of 135.59 on the social adj ustment subscale, which was significantly higher that the average score of 128.16 for students who were less satisfied (t = 2.605; p_< .05). Feeling that the ROSES program helped socially: Students who were more positive that the ROSES program had helped them socially scored an average of 136.37 on the social adjustment Subscale which was significantly higher than the average score of 128.88 achieved by the students who were less positive (t = 2.266; p < .05). 69 Hypothesis 5 is rejected. Feeling that the ROSES program had helped socially was significantly related to social adjustment (t = 2.266; p_< .05), as was satisfaction with the contact with a resident assistant (t = 2.605; p < .05). The two remaining social components of the ROSES program were not significantly related to social adjustment. Hyp_othesis 6: There is no difference in social adjustment scores, as measured by the social adjustment subscale of the SACQ, between students with a greater degree of participation in the social components indirectly related to the ROSES program and those with a lesser degree of participation. Prior knowledge of roommate: Students who knew their roommate prior to attending MSU had an average score of 142.31 on the social adjustment subscale, which was significantly higher that the average score of 131.55 for the students who did not know their roommate prior to attendance (t = 2.50; p_< .05). ‘ _S_afis_faction with roommate relationship: Students who were more satisfied with their roommate relationship scored an average of 135.47 on the social adjustment subscale, which was significantly higher than the average score of 128.16 for the students who were less satisfied with their roommate relationship (t = 2.037; p_< .05). Involvement: There was no significant difference in the social adjustment scores between students who reported being involved in non—instructional activities for three hours per week or more and those who were involved less than three hours per week (t = 1.336; p_> .05). 70 Hypothesis 6 is rejected. Two of the indirect components of the ROSES program were significantly related to social adjustment: knowing one’s roommate prior to attendance (t = 2.50; p_< .05); and satisfaction with one’s roommate relationship (t = 2.037; p_< .05). Involvement in non-curricular activities was not significantly related to social adjustment. Table 21 - T-tests for Participation with Social Components and Social Adjustment ROSES Social Component Adjustment x s.d. t prob. ROSES roommate Yes 132.55 22.30 No 137.43 18.18 1.27 .21 Proportion study with ROSES 2 .13 133.68 19.22 < .13 131.70 24.23 .597 .55 Satisfaction with RA 2 4 135.59 20.83 < 4 125.68 22.76 2.605 .01“ ROSES helped socially 2 4 136.37 20.62 < 4 128.88 22.55 2.266 03* Prior knowledge of roommate Yes 142.31 20.98 No 131.55 21.39 2.50 .02* Satisfaction with roommate 2 4 135.47 20.92 < 4 128.16 22.56 2.037 .04“ Hours per week involved 2 3 134.99 20.90 < 3 130.56 22.44 1.336 .18 *p_< .05 Hymthesis 7: There is no difference in social adjustment scores, as measured by the social adjustment subscale of the SACQ, between students with a greater degree of participation in any of the academic components directly related to the ROSES program and those with a lesser degree of participation. 71 Satisfaction with adviser: There was no significant difference in social adjustment scores between students who were more positive and those who were less positive about their satisfaction with the contact with an academic adviser (t = -.679; p > .05). F eeli_ngknown by the ROSES instructor: Students who felt known to a greater degree by their ROSES instructor scored an average of 136.23 on the social adjustment subscale which was significantly higher than the 128.53 scored by those who felt known to a lesser degree (t = 2.337; p < .05). Feeling that the ROSES progmn begged academically: There was no significant difference in adjustment scores between students who were most positive and those who were less positive in their feeling that the ROSES program had helped them academically (t = 1.738; p > .05). Hours per week in class: There was no significant difference in social adjustment scores between students who reported being in class 15 hours per week or more and those who were in class for less than 15 hours per week (t = .850; p_> .05). Miner week studying; There was no significant difference in social adjustment scores between students who reported studying 15 hours per week or more and those who studied less than 15 hours per week (t = .264; p_>.05). . Hypothesis 7 is rejected. The degree to which students felt known by their ROSES seminar instructor was significantly related to social adjustment (t = 2.337; 72 p < .05). None of the other academic components of the ROSES program were related to social adjustment. Table 22 presents the means for social adjustment for the two groups, as well as the results of t-tests for participation with the academic components and social adjustment. Table 22 - T-tests for Participation with Academic Components and Social Adjustment ROSES Social Component Adjustment x s.d. t prob. Satisfied with adviser 2 3 132.31 22.22 < 3 134.85 20.28 -.679 .50 Known by ROSES instructor 2 3 136.23 19.33 < 3 128.53 24.01 2.337 .02" ROSES helped academically 2 3 134.94 20.67 < 3 128.51 23.54 1.738 .09 Hoursperweekinclass 215 133.77 21.78 <15 130.84 21.63 .850 .40 Hours per week studying _>. 15 133.31 20.16 < 15 132.42 23.65 .264 .79 *p < .05 Full Adjustment To test the relationships between the ROSES components and overall adjustment to college, t-tests between the academic and social components, and full adjustment scores were conducted. Hypgthesis 8: There is no difference in full adjustment scores, as measured by the SACQ, between students with a greater degree of participation in any of the academic components directly related to the ROSES program and those with a lesser degree of participation. 73 The academic components directly related to the ROSES program are: satisfaction with an adviser, feeling known by the ROSES seminar instructor, and feeling that the ROSES program had helped academically. Satisfaction with adviser: There was no significant difference in full adjustment scores between those who were more satisfied and those who were less satisfied with the contact with an academic adviser (t = -. 1731 p_> .05). Feeling_kpown by the ROSES instructor: Students who reported a greater feeling of being known by their ROSES instructor achieved significantly higher full adjustment scores that students who reported feeling less well known by the ROSES instructor (t = 2.151; p_< .05). Feeling that the ROSES progrlim helped academically: There was no significant difference in full adjustment scores between students who were most positive and those who were less positive in their feeling that the ROSES program had helped them academically (t = 1.768; g> .05). Hypothesis 8 is rejected. Feeling known by the ROSES instructor was significantly related to full adjustment (t = 2.151; p < .05). The other two direct academic components were not related to full adjustment. W There is no difference in full adjustment scores, as measured by the SACQ, between students with a greater degree of participation in any of the academic components indirectly related to the ROSES program and those with a lesser degree of participation. 74 Academic components that are indirectly related to the ROSES program are the number of hours per week students report being in class, and the number of hours per week students report studying. Hoursy per weekfiin clas_s_: There was no significant difference in full adjustment scores for students who reported being in class 15 hours per week or more and those who reported being in class for less than 15 hours per week (t = 1.536; p_> .05). Hour; per week studying: There was no significant difference in full adjustment scores for students who reported studying 15 hours per week or more and those who reported studying less than 15 hours per week (t = 1.622; p_> .05). Hypothesis 9 is accepted. Neither the number of hours per week in class nor the hours per week spent studying was significantly related to full adjustment. Table 23 presents the means for full adjustment for the two groups, as well as the results of t-tests for participation with the academic components and full adjustment. 75 Table 23 - T-tests for Participation with Academic Components and Full Adjustment ROSES Full Component Adj ustment x s.d. t prob. Satisfied with adviser 2 3 417.82 61.36 < 3 419.73 60.87 -.173 .86 Known by ROSES instructor 2 3 426.94 59.76 < 3 406.92 61.32 2.151 03* ROSES helped academically Z 3 424.03 58.64 < 3 405.87 64.84 1.768 .08 Hours per week in class 215 423.59 62.42 <15 408.92 58.54 1.536 .13 Hours per week studying _>_ 15 425.47 58.13 < 15 410.28 63.96 1.622 .11 *g< .05 Hymthesis 10: There is no difference in full adjustment scores, as measured by the SACQ, between students with a greater degree of participation in any of the social components directly related to the ROSES program and those with a lesser degree of participation. The social components unique to the ROSES program were having a ROSES roommate, studying with other ROSES students, being satisfied with the contact with a resident assistant, and feeling that the ROSES program had helped socially (i.e. meeting people, getting involved). ROSES roommate: There was no significant difference in full adjustment scores between students who had roommates who were also participating in the ROSES program and those whose roommates were not in the ROSES program (t = 1.272; p_> .05). 76 Studw'ngth ROSES students: There was no significant difference in full adjustment scores between students who spent a greater proportion of their total study time studying with other ROSES students and those who studied less with other ROSES students (t = -.562; p_> .05). _Sat_r§f_‘action with resident aLssisaLnL; There was no significant difference in full adjustment scores between students who were more satisfied with the contact with a resident assistant and those who were less satisfied (t = 1.721; p_> .05). Feeling that the ROSES progr_am helped sociaflyy There was no significant difference in full adjustment scores between students who were more positive and those who were less positive in their feeling that the ROSES program had helped them socially (t = 1.333; p_> .05). Hypothesis 10 is accepted. None of the social components directly related to the ROSES program were significantly related to full adjustment. Hymthesis 11: There is no difference in full adjustment scores, as measured by the SACQ, between students with a greater degree of participation in any of the social components indirectly related to the ROSES program and those with a lesser degree of participation. The social components indirectly related to the ROSES program included knowing one’s roommate prior to attending MSU, satisfaction with the roommate relationship, satisfaction with the contact with a resident assistant, and the number of hours per week students were involved in non-instructional activities (i.e. student groups, work, community service). 77 Prior knowledge of roommat_e_: Students who knew their roommate prior to attending MSU scored an average of 449.93 on the full adjustment scale which was significantly higher than the 410.89 achieved by the students who did not know their roommate prior to attending MSU (t = -3. 102; p_< .05). §a11§faction with roommate relafionsLig There was no significant difference in full adjustment scores between students who were more satisfied with their roommate relationship and those who were less satisfied with their roommate relationship (t = .777; p> .05). Involvement: There was no significant difference in full adjustment scores between students who reported being involved in non-instructional activities for three hours per week or more and those who were involved less than three hours per week (t = .341; p_> .05). Hypothesis 11 is rejected. Knowing one’s roommate prior to attending MSU was significantly related to full adjustment (t = -3.102; p_< .05). None of the other indirect social components were related to full adjustment. Table 24 presents the means for full adj ustment for the two groups, as well as the results of t-tests for participation with the social components of the ROSES program and full adjustment. 78 Table 24 - T-tests for Participation with Social Components and Full Adjustment ROSES Full Component Adjustment x s.d. t prob. ROSES roommate Yes 430.43 60.31 No 414.90 61.38 1.272 .21 Proportion study with ROSES .>_ .13 415.87 59.32 <.13 421.16 63.67 -.562 .58 Satisfaction with RA 2 4 423.24 59.76 < 4 404.91 63.20 1.721 .09 ROSES helped socially 2 4 424.12 60.96 < 4 411.76 60.92 1.333 .18 Prior knowledge of roommate Yes 449.93 59.14 No 410.89 62.07 -3.102 .00" Satisfaction with roommate 2 4 420.89 60.58 <4 413.04 62.83 .777 .44 Hours per week involved .2 3 419.96 59.91 < 3 416.78 62.55 .341 .73 ** p_< .01 Fall Semester Grade Point Average To test for differences in fall semester grade point average, medians were used to divide respondents into two groups: those who had a greater degree of participation with the ROSES components and those who had a lesser degree of participation. Hypgthesis 12: There is no difference in fall grade point average between students with a greater degree of participation in any of the academic components directly related to the ROSES program and those with a lesser degree of participation. The academic components directly related to the ROSES program are: satisfaction with an adviser, feeling known by the ROSES seminar instructor, and feeling that the ROSES program had helped academically. 79 Swamion with adviser: There was no significant difference in fall semester grade point averages between students who were more satisfied with their contact with an academic adviser and those who were less satisfied (t = 1.687; p_> .05). Feelirygflown by ROSES instructor: There was no significant difference in fall semester grade point average for students who felt known to a greater degree by their ROSES instructor and those who felt known to a lesser degree (t = 1.425; p_> .05). Feeling ROSES helped academically: Students who were more positive in their feeling that the ROSES program had helped them socially earned an average fall semester grade point average of 2.95, which was significantly higher than the 2.67 earned by students who were less positive (t = 2.060; p_< .05). Hypothesis 12 is rejected. Feeling that the ROSES program helped academically was significantly related to fall semester grade point average (t = 2.060; p_< .05). However, the other two ROSES indicators were unrelated to grade point average. Hymthesis 13: There is no difference in fall grade point average between students with a greater degree of participation in any of the academic components indirectly related to the ROSES program and those with a lesser degree of participation. Academic components that are indirectly related to the ROSES program are the number of hours per week students report being in class, and the number of hours per week students report studying. 80 Hours in class: There was no significant difference in fall semester grade point average between students who spent 15 or more hours per week in class and those who spent less than 15 hours per week in class (t = 1.439; p_> .05). Hours studying: Students who spent 15 or more hours per week studying earned a fall semester grade point average of 3.01, which was significantly higher than the 2.70 grade point average earned by students who studied fewer than 15 hours per week (t = 2.705; p_< .05). Hypothesis 13 is rejected. Fall semester grade point average was significantly related to the number of hours per week students reported studying (t = 2.705; p_< .05). Table 25 presents the mean grade point averages for the two groups, as well as the results of t-tests for participation with the academic components of the ROSES program and fall semester grade point average. Table 25 - T-tests for Participation with Academic Components and Fall Semester GPA ROSES Fall GPA Component x s.d. t prob. Satisfied with adviser 2 3 2.92 .722 < 3 2.66 .893 1.687 .10 Known by ROSES instructor 2 3 2.93 .755 < 3 2.77 .784 1.425 .16 ROSES helped academically z 3 2.94 .701 < 3 2.67 .878 2.060 .04* Hours per week in class 2 15 2.95 .703 < 15 2.75 .868 1.439 .15 Hours per week studying 2 15 3.01 .745 < 15 2.70 .762 2.705 .01* *p_< .05 81 Hymthesis 14: There is no difference in fall grade point average between students with a greater degree of participation in any of the social components directly related to the ROSES program and those with a lesser degree of participation. The social components that were unique to the ROSES program were having a ROSES roommate, studying with other ROSES students, satisfaction with the contact with a resident assistant, and feeling that the ROSES program had helped socially (i.e. meeting people, getting involved). ROSES roommate: There was no significant difference in the fall semester grade point average for students who had a ROSES roommate and those who did not (t = -.666; p_> .05). Studyingwith ROSES students: There was no significant difference in the fall semester grade point average between students who spent 13 percent or more of their total study time studying with other ROSES students and those who studied with other ROSES students less than 13 percent ofthe time (t = -1.522; p_> .05). Satisfaction with resident assistant: There was no significant difference in the fall semester grade point average between students who were more satisfied with their contact with a resident assistant and those who were less satisfied (t = .325; p_> .05). Feeling that ROSES helped socially: There was no significance difference in the fall semester grade point average between students who were more positive in their feeling that the ROSES program had helped them socially and those who were less positive (t = .660; p_> .05). 82 Hypothesis 14 is accepted. There were no significant differences in fall semester grade point average between students who participated more in the social components of the ROSES program and those who participated less. Hymtbesis 15: There is no difference in fall grade point average between students with a greater degree of participation in any of the social components indirectly related to the ROSES program and those with a lesser degree of participation. The social components indirectly related to the ROSES program included knowing one’s roommate prior to attending MSU, satisfaction with the roommate relationship, and the number of hours per week students were involved in non- instructional activities (i.e. student groups, work, community service). Prior knowledge of roommjag There was no significant difference in the fall semester grade point average between students who knew their roommate prior to attending MSU and those who did not (t = 1.296; p_> .05). Satisfaction with roommafl There was no significant difference in the fall semester grade point average between students who were more satisfied with their roommate relationship and those who were less satisfied (t = -.059; p_> .05). Involvement: There was no significant difference in the fall semester grade point average between students who spent three or more hours per week involved in non-instructional activities and those who were involved fewer than three hours per week (t = .606; p_> .05). 83 Hypothesis 15 is accepted. There were no significant differences in fall semester grade point average between students who participated more in the social components indirectly related to the ROSES program and those who participated less. Table 26 presents the mean grade point averages for the two groups, as well as the results of t-tests for participation in the social components of the ROSES program and fall semester grade point average. Table 26 - T-tests for Participation with Social Components and Fall Semester GPA ROSES Fall GPA Component x s.d. t prob. ROSES roommate Yes 2.84 .811 No 2.92 .591 -.666 .51 Proportion study with ROSES 2.13 2.79 .685 <.13 2.96 .844 -1.522 .13 Satisfaction with RA 2 4 2.87 .752 < 4 2.83 .824 .325 .75 ROSES helped socially 2 4 2.91 .725 < 4 2.84 .777 .660 .51 Prior knowledge of roommate Yes 2.30 .606 No 2.82 .805 1.296 .20 Satisfaction with roommate 2 4 2.85 .793 < 4 2.86 .724 -.059 .95 Hours per week involved 2 3 2.88 .747 < 3 2.80 .833 .606 .55 Summary of Bivariate Analyses A summary of the results of bivariate tests of significance between the individual ROSES components and the outcomes of academic adjustment, social adjustment, full adjustment, and fall semester grade point average are presented in Table 27. An “*” indicates a significant relationship between the individual variable and outcome. 84 The analyses revealed that background variables were related to academic adjustment and fall semester grade point average. Gender was significantly related to academic adjustment and to fall gpa; ethnicity was significantly related to fall gpa; and predicted gpa was significantly related to academic adjustment and fall gpa. The only academic components of the ROSES program significantly related to academic adjustment were the number of hours per week spent in class and studying, both of which are not directly related to the ROSES program. The degree to which students felt known by their ROSES seminar instructor was significantly related to social adjustment and full adjustment. The degree to which students felt the ROSES program had helped them academically and the number of hours students reported studying were both significantly related to fall gpa. The social components of the ROSES program that were significantly. related to social adjustment were satisfaction with the contact with a resident assistant, feeling that ROSES had helped socially, knowing one’s roommate prior to attending MSU, and being satisfied with the roommate relationship. Knowing one’s roommate prior to attending MSU was also significantly related to academic adjustment and fiill adjustment. 85 Table 27 - Summary of Relationships between Components and Outcomes Academic Social Full Fall GPA BACKGROUND Gender * Ethnicity College Predicted GPA * * ACADEMIC Satisfied with Adviser Known by ROSES Instructor * * ROSES helped Academically Class (hours) Study (hours) * * SOCIAL ROSES roommate Study with ROSES Knew roommate * Satisfied with RA ROSES helped Socially Satisfied with Roommate Involvement (hours) Multiple Regression Analysis Multiple regression was utilized to test the full model, with all variables entered, for the outcomes of academic adjustment, social adjustment, and fall grade point average. Collinearity was assessed for all regression models by examining the tolerance statistics. All regression models had low tolerance statistics (close to 0); thus, multi-collinearity was not deemed problematic (George and Mallery, 1995). The same predictor variables were entered for each regression to determine the relative importance of each predictor for each of the four outcomes (academic 86 adjustment, social adjustment, full adjustment, and fall grade point average). Background variables in each regression included gender, race/ethnicity, college of enrollment, and predicted grade point average. The predicted grade point average is based upon high school grade point average, test scores (ACT or SAT), and quality of the high school attended. A second category of variables measured the academic components of the ROSES program and consisted of satisfaction with contact with an academic adviser; feeling known by the ROSES seminar instructor; the degree to which students felt the ROSES program had helped them academically; the number of hours per week respondents reported being in class; and the number of hours per week respondents reported studying. A third category of variables entered into the regression incorporated the social components of the ROSES program: having a ROSES student as a roommate; the proportion of study time students spent studying with other ROSES students; satisfaction with the contact with a resident assistant, the degree to which students felt the ROSES program had helped them socially; prior knowledge of their roommate; satisfaction with the roommate relationship; and the number of hours per week the student was engaged in non-instructional activities such as work, student organizations and community service. Finally, the ROSES seminar grade was entered into each regression equation. The seminar grade is based on the number of points earned by students for attendance and completion of assignments. Thus, the grade reflected the degree to which students attended and participated in the weekly ROSES seminar class and evening programs facilitated by the residence life staff. 87 Dummy variables were created for some of the variables. For gender, female was coded 1 and male was coded 0. A dummy variable was created for college of enrollment; with a code of 1 if the respondent had a major in the College of Engineering and a code of 0 if they were in the College of Natural Science or in the College of Agriculture and Natural Resources. Race/ethnicity was coded as a l to indicate non-minority students and 0 to indicate students in all other racial categories. Having a ROSES roommate was coded 1, while having a roommate who was not a ROSES student was coded 0. Knowing one’s roommate prior to attending MSU was coded 1, and not knowing one’s roommate prior to attendance was coded O. Satisfaction scores (satisfaction with the roommate, satisfaction with the Resident Assistant and satisfaction with the academic adviser) were coded 0 through 5, with 0 being “does not apply” and 5 indicating “very satisfied”. How well the respondent felt the ROSES seminar instructor knew them was coded 1 for “not at all” through 5 for “very well”. The degree to which students believed the ROSES program had helped them socially and academically was coded from 1 for “helped not at all” to 5 for “helped to a great extent”. The number of hours each week that respondents reported studying with other ROSES students was expressed as a proportion of the total number of hours each respondent reported studying. Multiple regression analyses were conducted for three different outcomes: academic adjustment, social adjustment, and fall semester grade point average. The same variables were entered into each of the regressions. Of particular interest was the amount of variance in the outcome variable explained by the predictor variables. The unstandardized regression coefficients (B) indicate the effect of each individual variable on the outcome variable. Additionally, by examining the relationships between 88 individual predictor variables and the outcome, insight was gained with regard to the relative importance of various predictors to that outcome. Because the independent variables had different units of measurement, the Beta coefficients were used for comparative purposes. All regression results were interpreted at the .05 level of significance, but .05 - .10 was also examined for marginally significant results. Academic Adjustment As shown in Table 28, background variables, social component variables, academic component variables, and the ROSES seminar grade, taken together; accounted for 20% of the variance in academic adjustment scores. This relationship was statistically significant (F17343 = 2.08, p_< .05). In this regression model, the ROSES seminar grade was significantly positively related to academic adjustment (t 17,143 = 2.00, p < .05). Knowing one’s roommate prior to attending MSU was marginally significant (67,143 = 1.93; p < .10). For each point increase in the seminar grade, the academic adjustment score increased by 5.35 points. Knowing one’s roommate prior to attendance added 10.01 points to the academic adjustment score. 89 Table 29 - Regression Coefficients for Model Predicting Academic Adjustment Predictor Variables B Beta t Sig._ (Constant) 82.31 4. 12 .00 Engineering (dummyy 8.071 .146 1.485 .14 Ethnicity (dummy) .501 .008 .093 .93 Gender (dummy) 7.231 .149 1.567 . 12 Predicted GPA 4.159 .054 .627 .53 Know roommate (dummy) 10.006 .165 1.933 .06+ ROSES roommate (dummy) -2.516 -.042 -.492 .62 Involvement (hours per week) -.301 -.O98 -1.223 .22 Study with ROSES 4.974 .037 .449 .65 Satisfied with RA -.983 -.055 -.682 .50 Satisfied with roommate .606 .030 .373 .71 ROSES helped socially .594 .033 .336 .74 Study (hours per week) .354 .128 1.517 .13 Satisfied with adviser .587 .038 .457 .65 Known by ROSES instructor 1.059 .046 .542 .59 ROSES helped academically 1.175 .061 .630 .53 Class (hours pg week) .299 .052 .649 .52 Seminar grade 5.349 .184 2.004 .05* F17,143 = 2.08; p < .05; R2 = .20 *p_< .05; ’p_< .10 Social Adjustment All variables in the model accounted for 21% of the variance in social adjustment scores, and were significantly associated with social adjustment (F1734; = 2.24; g< .05). The degree of satisfaction with one’s roommate (Q7343 = 2.05; p< .05) and the number of hours spent in class 017,143 = 2.54; p_< .05) were both significantly positively related to social adjustment. The regression model is presented in Table 30. Each incremental increase in satisfaction with one’s roommate added 3.07 points to the social adjustment score. For each additional hour spent in class, social adjustment scores increased 1.08 points. 90 Table 30 - Regression Coefficients for Model Predicting Social Adjustment Predictor variables B Beta t Sig._ (Constant) 108.437 5.904 .00 Egineering (dummy) 5.189 .101 1.038 .30 Ethnicity (dummy) -1.076 -.018 -.218 .83 Gender (dummy) 6.746 .150 1.590 .1 1 Predicted GPA -8.961 -.125 -1.469 .14 Know roommate (dummy) 7.1 18 .127 1.496 .14 ROSES roommate (dummy) -3.463 -.062 -.737 .46 Involvement (hours per week) .292 .103 1.292 .20 Study with ROSES 13.553 .108 1.330 .19 Satisfied with RA 1.501 .091 1.132 .26 Satisfied with roommate 3.070 .164 2.053 .04" ROSES helped socially 2.620 .157 1.612 .11 Study (hours per week) -.073 -.028 -.341 .73 Satisfied with adviser —1.023 -.O72 -.868 .39 Known by ROSES instructor 2.573 .121 1.433 . 15 ROSES helped academically -.658 -.O37 -.383 .70 Class (hours per week) 1.077 .203 2.542 .01“ Seminar grade -.583 -.022 -.238 .81 Fm” = 2.24; p_< .05; R2 = .21 *p < .05 Full Adjustment The regression model including background variables, social component variables, academic component variables, and the ROSES seminar grade explained 17% of the variance in full adjustment scores, and the overall relationship was significant (F17343 = 1.78; p_< .05). Hours spent in class (67,143 = 2.10; p_< .05) and knowing one’s roommate (67,143 = 2.27; p_< .05) were significantly positively related to full adjustment. For each hour spent in class, full adjustment score increased 2.61 points. Knowing one’s roommate added 31.58 points to the full adjustment score. The final regression model is presented in Table 31. 91 Table 31 - Regression Coefficients for Model Predicting Full Adjustment Predictor Variables B Beta t 8L (Constant) 289.343 5.376 .00 Engineering (dummy) 15.886 .108 1.085 .28 Ethnicity (dummy) 3.649 .021 .253 .80 Gender (dummy) 21.109 .164 1.698 .09 Predicted GPA -5.422 -.026 -.303 .76 Know roommate gummy) 31.578 .196 2.265 .03* ROSES roommate (dummy) -5.805 -.037 -.422 .67 Involvement (hours per week -.093 -.011 -. 141 .89 Study with ROSES 19.006 .053 .637 .53 Satisfied with RA 1.193 .025 .307 .76 Satisfied with roommate 5.618 .105 1.282 .20 ROSES helped socially 5.274 .110 1.108 .27 Study (hours per week) .315 .043 .502 .62 Satisfied with adviser -1.673 -.O41 -.484 .63 Known byROSES instructor 4.388 .072 .834 .41 ROSES helped academically -.650 -.013 -. 129 .90 Class (houraper weeky 2.605 .171 2.098 .04“ Seminar grade 6.675 .086 .928 .36 F1734; = 1.78; p_< .05; RI: .17 *p < .05 Fall Semester Grade Point Average Fall semester grade point average was the final outcome assessed by multiple regression. Because the seminar grade was included in the calculation of the fall semester grade point average, in this analysis, fall gpa was recalculated to exclude the seminar grade. Background variables, social component variables, academic component variables, and the ROSES seminar grade, taken together, accounted for 42% of the variation in fall semester grade point average. The model was also statistically significant (R7343 = 5.973; p_< .01). Predicted grade point average was significantly positively related to fall semester grade point average (tum = 4.21; p_< .01), with each point increase in predicted grade point average adding an additional .31 point to the fall 92 semester grade point average. The other significant individual variable in this model was the ROSES seminar grade (67,143 = 4.273; p<.01), with each point increase in the ROSES seminar grade increasing the fall semester grade point average by .33 point. The coefficients for the final regression model are presented in Table 32. Table 32 - Regression Coefficients for Model Predicting Fall Semester GPA Predictor Variables B Beta t Sig._ (Consan -2.007 -3.291 .00 Engineering (dummy) .221 .112 1.347 .18 Ethnicity (dummy) .181 .078 1.121 .26 Gender @ummy) .177 . 103 1.275 .20 Predicted GPA .859 .312 4.211 00* Know roommate (dummy) -.024 -.011 -. 152 .88 ROSES roommate (dummy) -.073 -.034 -.471 .64 Involvement (hours per week) .007 .033 .477 .63 Study with ROSES .264 .055 .785 .43 Satisfied with RA .017 .027 .396 .69 Satisfied with roommate .066 .092 1.341 .18 ROSES helped socially -.058 -.O91 -1.089 .28 Study (hours per wecfi .004 .039 .546 .59 Satisfied with adviser .043 .078 1.099 .27 Known by ROSES instructor .041 .050 .695 .49 ROSES helped academically .059 .087 1.053 .29 Class (hours per week) .009 .045 .658 .51 Seminar grade .344 .334 4.273 .00“ F1114, = 5.973; p< .01; R2 = .42 *p < .05 Summary Bivariate analyses identified significant relationships between individual variables and outcomes. Regression analyses tested each hypothesis using the full set of predictor variables. 93 Academic Adjustment For academic adjustment, the seminar grade (Beta = .184) was a stronger predictor than knowing one’s roommate prior to attendance (Beta = . 165) though they were both significant in the regression. Neither of these variables were academic components of the ROSES program. With all variables included in the model, no other variables were significant predictors of academic adjustment. Social Adjustment The hours spent in class (Beta = .203) was a stronger predictor of social adjustment than the degree of satisfaction with the roommate relationship (Beta = .164). With all variables included in the regression, these two variables were the only ones which significantly predicted social adjustment. Both of these variables were considered components of the ROSES program, albeit indirect; that is, they are not exclusive components of the ROSES program. Only the degree of satisfaction with the roommate relationship was a direct social component. Full Adjustment With all variables entered in the regression, only knowing one’s roommate prior to attendance at MSU and the number of hours per week spent in class were significant predictors of full adjustment. Prior knowledge of one’s roommate (Beta = .196) was a stronger predictor than the number of hours per week in class (Beta = .171). Neither of these variables are uniquely related to the ROSES program. Fall Semester Grade Point Average With all variables entered in the regression, only the predicted grade point average and seminar grade were significant predictors of fall semester grade point 94 average. The seminar grade (Beta = .427) was a stronger predictor than the predicted grade point average (Beta = .323). None of the academic components or social components of the ROSES program were significant predictors of fall semester grade point average. Table 32 presents a summary of the relationships between the predictor variables and the outcomes of academic adjustment, social adjustment, full adjustment, and fall semester grade point average. The bivariate relationships revealed that individual academic components of the ROSES program were related to academic adjustment, full adjustment, and to fall semester grade point average; but not to social adjustment. Some of the individual social components of the ROSES program were related to social adjustment and fiill adjustment; but not to academic adjustment or to fall semester grade point average. The results of the regression analyses revealed that with background variables, academic components, social components, and seminar grade entered into the model, the significant predictors of academic adjustment were the seminar grade and knowing one’s roommate prior to attendance at MSU. The significant predictors of social adjustment, with all variables entered into the regression, were the hours per week spent in class and satisfaction with one’s roommate relationship. The significant predictors of fiill adjustment were the number of hours per week in class and knowing one’s roommate prior to attendance at MSU. With all variables entered into the regression, the significant predictors of fall semester grade point average were the seminar grade and the predicted grade point average. 95 Table 32 - Summary of Relationships between Predictors and Outcomes Academic Social Full Fall GPA BACKGROUND Gender * Ethnicity College Predicted GPA *X ACADEMIC Satisfied with Adviser Known by ROSES Instructor ROSES helped Academically Class (hours) Study (hours) SOCIAL ROSES roommate Study with ROSES Satisfied with RA ROSES helped Socially Knew roommate *X *X Satisfied with Roommate *X Involvement (hours) ROSES Seminar X Note. * = significant bivariate relationship X = significant predictor of outcome 96 Chapter 5 SUMMARY, DISCUSSION, AND RECOMMENDATIONS Summary and Discussion The purpose of this study was to examine the contribution of a residential program for first year science and engineering students on their adjustment to college. Specifically, social and academic aspects of the program were examined to determine their relationship with academic adj ustment, social adjustment, full adjustment, and fall semester grade point average. All first year students (221 total) enrolled in the Residential Option for Science and Engineering Students (ROSES) were included in this study. All respondents received an introductory letter and two surveys near the end of fall semester, 1997. This time was selected in order to give students nearly a full semester of experiences in college and the ROSES program, yet not conflict with the end of the semester and finals week. A total of 199 surveys were returned, representing a 90% return rate. Of the returned surveys, 174 were complete and used for statistical analyses, yielding a 79% usable return rate. Data were analyzed using the SPSS for Windows Statistical Package for the Social Sciences. Two primary research questions guided the study: 1) Do components of the ROSES program contribute differentially to academic adjustment, social adjustment, full adjustment, and fall semester grade point average; and 2) Which individual components of the ROSES program make the most significant contributions to predicting adjustment to college and fall semester grade point average? Bivariate relationships between individual components of the ROSES program and outcomes were examined. Multiple 97 regression was utilized to identify significant predictors of academic adjustment, social adjustment, full adjustment and fall semester grade point average. Data were analyzed at the .05 level of significance, with .05 - .10 also examined in the multiple regression analyses for marginally significant results. Relationships between Individual Variables and Outcomes Background Variables Background variables consisted of gender, ethnicity, college of enrollment, and predicted grade point average. Females achieved significantly higher academic adjustment than males. Females also had significantly higher fall semester grade point averages than males, even though there was no significant difference in the predicted grade point averages between females and males. This finding is interesting because females are often thought to achieve lower grades in math and science. The first semester curriculum for all students consists of general education course requirements. Included in the typical first semester course schedule for entering science and engineering students are introductory math and science courses. Further research should explore whether females who participated in the ROSES program as first-year students continue to earn higher grades in math, science, and engineering courses during subsequent academic years. Nonetheless, this finding for female students in the ROSES program should not be overlooked in the male-dominated fields of science and engineering. Ethnicity was significantly related to fall semester grade point average. Minority students in the ROSES program had significantly lower grade point averages than non- minority students, despite entering college with predicted grade point averages that were not significantly different. This finding is troubling, not only for the success of minority 98 students, but also as it relates to the under-representation of minority group members in science and engineering fields. Predicted grade point average was significantly related to academic adj ustment and to fall semester grade point average. There were no significant relationships between the college of enrollment and academic adjustment, social adjustment, full adjustment, or fall semester grade point average. Academic Variables Not surprisingly, the number of hours students reported being in class and the number of hours students reported studying were significantly related to academic adjustment. Additionally, the number of hours students reported studying was significantly related to fall semester grade point average. These relationships are reasonable to understand. Faculty and staff should certainly use this information to promote class attendance and studying, especially among new students. Satisfaction with the contact with an academic adviser was not related to any form of adjustment or to fall semester grade point average. This finding is unexpected, given the intentional efforts to connect students with academic advisers. The advisers often meet the ROSES students during the summer orientation program. Once school starts, academic advisers for the ROSES students attempt various strategies to maintain on- going contact with students throughout the first semester. Some students are required to meet with their academic adviser as part of the ROSES seminar, and some advisers serve as instructors for the ROSES seminar. Feeling known by the ROSES seminar instructor was not related to academic adjustment or fall semester grade point average, but was significantly related to social 99 adj ustment and full adjustment. It is possible that, for those advisers who are ROSES seminar instructors, students relate to them more as instructors than as advisers. In any case, the quality of the relationship between students and the instructors for the seminar appears to be important to social adjustment and full adjustment. Students’ feelings that the ROSES program helped them academically (i.e. classes, studying) was positively related to fall semester grade point average. This finding could also be explained by other factors (such as motivation or general attitude toward academic work) which could contribute to both. Social Variables Knowing one’s roommate prior to attending MSU was significantly related to academic, social, and full adjustment. Further, being satisfied with one’s roommate relationship was significantly related to social adjustment. These findings suggest that comfort, or at least the lack of uncertainty about one’s roommate, may be beneficial, not only to social adjustment, but also to academic and full adjustment. It is reasonable that the quality of the roommate relationship may provide elements of the social validation that Terenzini et a1. (1992) identified as crucial to the transition to college. However, neither of these variables (prior knowledge of roommate or the satisfaction with the roommate relationship) are directly related to the ROSES program, and there is nothing to suggest that the importance of roommate relationships is unique to the ROSES program. An unexpected finding was that having a roommate who was also a ROSES student was not related to social adjustment or to fall semester grade point average. It was hypothesized, and implied in the design of the ROSES program, that students would 100 benefit from having roommates who were in the same program and going through similar experiences. It may be that any effects of having a ROSES roommate are evident later in one’s academic experience. The proportion of time students spent studying with other ROSES students was not related to academic adjustment, social adjustment, full adjustment, or to grade point average. One of the underlying premises of the ROSES program was that students would find academic support and assistance from one another that might assist them academically and socially. For the group of students in this study, only about 17% of their total study time was spent with other ROSES students. It was anticipated that students in the ROSES program would spend a greater proportion of their total study time with one another because they have several of the same courses. This finding is difficult to interpret due to the lack of a comparison group (students in the same classes but who do not live in the same residence hall). It could be that studying together 17% of the time is more than other groups of first-year students study together. In any case, studying with other ROSES students was not significantly related to adjustment or to fall semester grade point average. Satisfaction with the contact with a resident assistant was related to social adjustment. As returning students, resident assistants are in positions to be viewed by students as institutional agents who are also students. Resident assistants interpret the rules and procedures of the institution, assist individual students with personal issues, and plan activities. In the ROSES program, the resident assistants are involved with the new student orientation program and the evening programs which supplement the ROSES seminar class. Thus, students have opportunities to see resident assistants often and in a 101 variety of roles. The resident assistants may act as peer socialization agents in the living environment. Involvement in non-instructional activities did not relate to any form of adjustment or grade point average. It may be that students need a longer period of time than what was afforded in this study in order to establish meaningful involvement opportunities. Further investigation of the role of involvement in non-instructional activities and adjustment over a longer period of time may be valuable. The feeling that the ROSES program helped students socially (i.e. meeting people, getting involved) was significantly related to social adjustment. This finding, in combination with the significance of the relationships with the roommate and resident assistant, suggests at least an initial importance of the social and interpersonal experiences for new students. Predictors of Adjustment and Fall Semester Grade Point Average The second research question of interest in this study was to identify the components of the ROSES program which predicted academic adjustment, social adjustment, full adjustment, and fall semester grade point average. Multiple regression was used to identify the significant predictors for each outcome. Academic Adjustment Only one of the variables found to be significantly related to academic adjustment in the bivariate analyses was identified by multiple regression as a significant predictor of academic adjustment. Knowing one’s roommate prior to attending MSU was considered a social component; yet, it was a significant predictor of academic adjustment. This 102 finding was surprising and suggests the importance of the social environment, not only for social adjustment, but also for academic adjustment. The ROSES seminar grade was also a significant predictor of academic adjustment. This finding seems congruent with the topics that are covered in the seminar class and evening sessions. Topics include academic skills such as test taking and time management, as well as issues such as learning styles and career exploration. Students are also exposed to a variety of academic resources on campus. Perhaps most interesting is the finding that none of the variables identified in this study as academic components of the ROSES program were significant predictors of academic adjustment. Despite the efforts of advisers and ROSES seminar instructors to develop connections with new students, the two measures in this study (satisfaction with the contact with an adviser and feeling known by a ROSES instructor) were not significant predictors of academic adjustment. Even though the number of hours spent in class and the number of hours spent studying were significantly related to academic adjustment in the bivariate analyses, neither were identified as significant predictors in the regression analysis. Overall, the results of the regression analysis do not suggest that greater involvement with the academic components of the ROSES program results in higher academic adjustment. Prior knowledge of one’s roommate (identified in this study as a social component) is indirectly related to the ROSES program in that knowing one’s roommate is not a design element of the ROSES program. The only significant predictor of academic adjustment that is specifically related to the ROSES program is the ROSES seminar class. 103 Social Adjustment The number of hours students reported being in class was a significant predictor of social adjustment. This finding was surprising in that the number of hours per week spent in class was considered to be an academic component. One possible explanation for this finding is that being in class may increase one’s interactions with others (peers and faculty), which may contribute to social adjustment. Satisfaction with the relationship with one’s roommate was significantly related to social adjustment and was also a significant predictor of social adjustment. Satisfaction with the roommate relationship is not directly related to the ROSES program, but in this study, it was considered a social component. This finding seems to highlight the importance of the immediate environment (one’s room) for the successful social adj ustment of new students. None of the other individual variables of the ROSES program were significant predictors of social adjustment. Relationships with peers in the enviromnent, specifically with other ROSES students and with resident assistants, were not predictors of social adjustment. Thus, the hypothesis that greater involvement in the social components of the ROSES program results in higher social adjustment is only minimally supported, since the only significant predictors of social adjustment were indirectly related to the ROSES program. Full Adjustment Knowing one’s roommate prior to attendance at MSU and the number of hours per week students reported spending in class were significant predictors of full adjustment. Again, the importance of the roommate relationship is crucial. The positive 104 relationship between the number of hours spent in class and full adjustment may suggest that the structure of attending class and the contact with others are important to overall adjustment. Neither of the significant predictors of full adjustment are features directly related to the ROSES program. Fall Semester Grade Point Average Regression analysis identified the predicted grade point average as the only background variable that was a significant predictor of fall grade point average, despite the significant relationships between gender and fall grade point average and between ethnicity and fall grade point average. Thus, with all other components of the ROSES program considered, gender and ethnicity are not significant predictors of fall grade point average. The ROSES seminar grade was also a significant predictor of fall semester grade point average. Because the grade is based upon class attendance, evening session attendance, and completion of assignments, it could be that the student habits required for successful seminar performance are also practiced in other classes, which contributes to successful academic performance in other courses. It was hypothesized that the social components would not be significantly related to fall semester grade point average. In the bivariate analyses, the number of hours of studying and feeling that the ROSES program had helped academically were both significantly related to fall semester grade point average. However, none of the academic components or social components of the ROSES program were identified by multiple regression as significant predictors of fall semester grade point average. 105 Summag Even though there were some significant relationships between some individual variables and outcomes, multivariate analyses did not identify the significant predictors that had been hypothesized. Academic components did not significantly predict academic adj ustment or fall semester grade point average. The only social component that significantly predicted social adjustment was the degree of satisfaction with the roommate relationship, which is not a directly related component of the ROSES program. Implications for Practice The findings of this study, both the expected and the unexpected, provide implications for staff and institutional leaders. Implications for Staff One striking implication of this study is the importance of the roommate relationship on adjustment to college. This may be true for students not only in a residential program such as the ROSES program, but for students in other programs or in no special program. Thus, it seems wise for administrators of living-learning programs and housing systems to allow students to choose their roommates; and for residence hall staff to focus on the development of satisfactory roommate relationships. Certainly, many students enter college without the opportunity or inclination to live with someone they already know. Residence life staff should consider implementing strategies which would more quickly assist roommates in establishing roommate relationships that are comfortable. It also seems that living-learning programs, such as the ROSES program, are designed based upon knowledge or beliefs that may not be fully understood or tested. 106 For example, there appears to be support for the idea of housing students together who have classes or majors in common. Presumably, this arrangement should make it easier for students to study with one another. Yet, in this study, the proportion of time students spent studying with other ROSES students did not significantly relate to adjustment or to fall semester grade point average, nor did it significantly predict academic or social adjustment or fall grade point average. Perhaps additional or different interventions are necessary to achieve the maximum benefit from housing students together who have classes or majors in common. It may be that students do not know how to productively study with others. Instructors should consider designing assignments that will assist students in developing the skills necessary for working with others to complete a task. Another benefit would be that such group work may reduce the degree of competiveness among students that can be detrimental to student success. Implications for Institutional Leaders For leaders of institutions, especially of the size and complexity of Michigan State University, the importance of the student transition experience should not be overlooked. This study suggests that for students in their first semester of college, the immediate and interpersonal environment is important. In a very general sense, it appears that those students who form satisfactory relationships with those nearest to them in their environment experience greater social adjustment. The small classroom experience of the ROSES seminar allows students to have contact with an adult who acts as an agent of the institution. Yet the relationships with advisers and ROSES instructors, as measured in this study, do not significantly contribute to adjustment or fall semester grade point average. The allocation of resources so that 107 first-year students in the ROSES program have a small classroom experience is well— intentioned, but should be examined further to determine the relative costs in terms of resources and benefits to students. It may also be important to note that the academic advisers for the ROSES program are professional advisers and not faculty. Faculty advisers may be perceived differently by students and their impact on students may be different than that of professional advisers. Assigning faculty advisers to first-year students is a significant institutional resource, but one that may be worth exploring. Leaders should continue to encourage and support the collaborative efforts between academic affairs and student affairs which are necessary to implement living- leaming programs. New programs should be designed to address issues or deficiencies identified through careful study, with the goals of the programs clearly articulated. Additionally, systematic assessment must be conducted to determine the contributions of living-learning programs and to guide the innovations made to existing programs. Assessment and evaluation data should be used to determine the appropriate roles and responsibilities of academic and student affairs staff members. Finally, many living-learning programs focus on experiences for first-year students. Without diminishing the attention to first-year students, institutional leaders ought to encourage faculty and staff to consider what happens to students beyond their first year, with special attention to the sophomore year. Do living-learning programs for first-year students create a set of expectations about college that lead to disillusionment once the first year program has ended? Are students who enter the institution through a smaller, more comfortable living-learning program ready and able to negotiate the larger university, or do they experience another transition? 108 Limitations of the Study A limitation of this study is the reliance on self-reported information about participation and involvement with components of the ROSES program, as well as self- reported responses to the items on the adjustment surveys. Adjustment scores from this group of respondents were similar to scores obtained in other research studies (see Baker & Siryk, 1989), so there is some assurance of the use of this survey for this purpose. Additionally, the elements that compose the ROSES program are difficult to isolate and measure as individual components. The respondents in this study were not a randomly drawn sample from the general student population; thus, the results can not be generalized to other groups of first-year students. There is a self-selection factor among these respondents in that students interested in science and engineering majors request to participate in the ROSES program; thus, they may exhibit a higher level of motivation, readiness, and interest in college experiences than peers who do not express interest in this program. Additionally, due to space limitations in the program, two of the participating colleges establish selection criteria for students interested in the ROSES program. It may be interesting to examine the effects of a living-learning program on the adjustment and academic success of students who are less academically prepared. Finally, participation in an academic, residential program is only one set of a multitude of experiences which influence and shape the lives of college students. This study did not attempt to measure or to control for the myriad of academic and social experiences which are potentially related to adjustment to college or to fall semester 109 grade point average. This study also did not control for personal or psychological factors within individual students which may be related to adj ustment. Recommendations for Further Research This study was primarily interested in outcomes of adjustment to college and the contribution of a residential program on those outcomes. Other research about college students suggests that the first six weeks are crucial in the transition to college. However, ‘ Baker and Siryk (1989) report some significant effects on at least some of the adjustment I scales depending on whether the survey was administered during the first semester or the second semester. Therefore, it would be beneficial to examine the contributions of the ROSES program to adjustment over a longer period of time. For example, it may be that the relationships formed with peers in the first year of college will lead to less competition among students when they are engaged in upper level science or engineering courses. Controlled, longitudinal studies should be undertaken to explore whether students who participate in the ROSES program as first-year students progress differently through their majors and ultimately to graduation. Given the factors which contribute to students switching out of science and engineering majors (Seymour, 1992), it would be insightful to investigate major- switching among students who enter the university as ROSES students. While retention within science and engineering majors is a desired outcome of the ROSES program, there will undoubtedly be students who switch to non-science and non-engineering majors. It would be interesting to know if ROSES students who ultimately switch into other majors cite different reasons for switching than those identified by Seymour’s research. 110 This study raises questions about the impacts of clustering students with similar academic interests in one residence hall. While this study did not compare students in this hall-based program to similar students not participating in a similar program, the added residential features of this program did not seem to have powerful predictability relative to adjustment. Further research which compares participants of living-learning programs to non-participants is needed to further investigate the added value of the residential experience. It is also possible that living-learning programs associated with different academic disciplines would yield a different array of relationships between the program components, adjustment, and grade point average. Certainly, multi-faceted adjustment to college and fall semester grade point average are only a few of the potentially valuable outcomes of a living-learning program. Additional research ought to examine relationships between participating in these sorts of programs and other outcomes associated with higher education, including cognitive and intellectual development, psychosocial development, and educational attainment. It would also be valuable to conduct qualitative research with students in living-learning programs in order to describe the kinds of experiences that students identify as meaningful to their adjustment and success. Current conversations about undergraduate education are laced with suggestions for programs similar to the ROSES program. Living-learning programs, learning communities, and freshman interest groups are intended to add integration and coherence to the undergraduate experience. These programs are designed to increase the connections between curricular and co-curricular aspects of the collegiate experience. Program developers must be explicit about the program goals and implement strategies 111 consistent with the goals and mission of the program. Even when conceptually sound, these programs require resources of time, effort, and money. Evaluation reseach should be conducted to determine the effectiveness and efficiency of these various innovations, and the degree to which the desired goals are met. Additionally, program developers must be cognizant of the effects of such programs on students from diverse backgrounds and in different academic disciplines. 112 APPENDIX A APPENDIX A SCIENCE AND ENGINEERING BACCALAUREATE DEGREE PROGRAMS MICHIGAN STATE UNIVERSITY College of Agriculture and Natural Resources Agribusiness Management Agriculture and Natural Resources Communications Agriscience Animal Science Biosystems Engineering Building Construction Management Crop and Soil Science Environmental and Natural Resource Policy Issues Fisheries and Wildlife Food Industry Management Food Science Forestry Horticulture Packaging Park, Recreation, and Tourism Resources Public Resource Management College of Engineering Biosystems Engineering Chemical Engineering Civil Engineering Computer Science Computer Engineering Electrical Engineering Engineering Arts Material Science and Engineering Manufacturing Engineering Mechanical Engineering Mechanics 113. College of Natural Science Astrophysics Biochemistry Biochemistry/Biotechnology Botany and Plant Pathology Chemical Physics Chemistry Clinical Laboratory Science Computational Mathematics Entomology Environmental Geoscience Geological Sciences Mathematics Medical Technology Microbiology Physics Physiology Statistics and Probability Zoology 114 APPENDIX B APPENDIX B SURVEY ITEM CLUSTERS Survey items on the Student Adaptation to College Questionnaire (SACQ) are organized into 12 critical clusters, which comprise four subscales. ACADEMIC ADJUSTMENT SUBSCALE Cluster Item Description ls definite about reasons for being in college. Has well-defined academic goals. Motivation Considers college degree important. Doubts value of college degree. Enjoys academic work. Most interests are not related to course work. Keeps up-to-date with academic work. Application Does not work as hard as he or she should. Is not motivated to study. Attends classes regularly. Finds academic work difficulty. Does not function well during exams. Is satisfied with academic performance. Performance Does not feel smart enough for course work. Does not use study time efficiently. Enjoys writing papers for courses. Has trouble concentrating when studying. Does not do well academically, considering effort. Has trouble getting started on homework. Is satisfied with variety of courses. Is satisfied with quality of courses. Academic Environment ls satisfied with program of courses. Is satisfied with professors. Is satisfied with academic situation. 115 APPENDIX B (cont’d). SOCIAL ADJUSTMENT SUBSCALE Cluster Item Description Fits in well with college environment. Is very involved with college social activities. General Is adjusting well to college. Has several close social ties. Is satisfied with social participation. Is satisfied with social life. Is meeting people and making friends. Has informal contact with professors. Gets along well with roommates. Has difficulty feeling at ease with others at Other PeOple college. Does not mix well with opposite sex. Feels different from others in undesirable ways. Has good friends to talk about problems with. Is lonesome for home. Nostalgia Feels lonely a lot. Would rather be home. Is pleased about decision to attend this Social Environment college. Enjoys living in a dormitory. Is satisfied with extracurricular activities. 116 APPENDIX B (cont’d). PERSONAL-EMOTIONAL SUBSCALE Cluster Item Description Feels tense or nervous. Feels blue and moody. Being independent has not been easy. Is not able to control emotions well lately. Psychological Has thought about seeking psychological help recently. Gets aneg too easily lately. Sometimes things gets muddled too easily. Worries a lot about college expenses. Has trouble coping with college stress. Feels tired a lot lately. Appetite is good. Has a lot of headaches. Physical Gained or lost a lot of weight lately. Is not sleeping well. Feels in good health. ATTACHMENT SUBSCALE Cluster Item Description Is pleased with decision to go to college. Thinks a lot about dropping of college General permanently. Is thinking about taking time off from college. Is pleased about attending this college. Would prefer to be at another college. This College Expects to finish bachelor’s degree. Is thinking about transferring to another college. 117 APPENDIX C APPENDIX C ROSES EXPERIENCES SURVEY Please answer the following questions based on your experiences this semester. There are no correct or incorrect answers. By providing your PID, you authorize the researcher to use selected institutional data (fall semester course enrollment, ROSES seminar attendance, ACT/SAT score, high school F, GPA, enrollment status for spring semester, expected fall semester GPA, fall semester ' GPA, academic status at the beginning of spring semester) for research purposes only. Information from this survey will be used for research purposes only. Research findings will not associate individual students with specific responses. :.—-.II.'-§. nl'T‘l'l" :- v .' Your PID: 1. How many roommates do you have (or for a majority of the semester)? zero one two (if zero, skip to item 2) a. Do you have a roommate(s) in the ROSES program? yes no b. Did you know your roommate(s) before coming to MSU? yes no 2. For the following activities, indicate the approximate numbers of hours per week you have been involved in each activity this semester. You can round your answers to the nearest whole hour. If you have had no involvement, place a zero in the space provided. Hours per week Activity Hall groups (caucus, government, floor council, other groups) Campus organizations (i.e. academic or social organizations) Part-time work Off-campus organizations (i.e. religious or community groups) Community service (volunteer work) Floor activities (i.e. IM sports, social activities) Class Study (i.e. homework, reading for class, other academic work) :qcrhor-Log‘e 118 3. Of the total hours you study, how many hours per week do you study: a. in your own room b. in another student room in your residence hall c. in a consulting room in Bailey Hall (B108, B208, B308) d. in a lounge in your residence hall e. in a location other than any residence hall 4. Of the total hours you study, how many hours per week do you study: a. by yourself b. with other ROSES student(s) c. with non-ROSES student(s) d. with a consultant in Bailey Hall For the following items, please indicate the number of times you have interacted with the individuals listed. If you have had no contact, please write a zero in the space provided. 5. How many times this semester have you: __ a. met with a consultant for ROSES students in Bailey Hall _ b. talked with your academic adviser __ c. talked with an instructor (professor, TA, ROSES seminar instructor) outside of class 6. How many times this semester have you talked about academics (i.e. classes, grades, professors, major, studying) with: a. your academic adviser b. an instructor (professor, TA) c. a peer leader (1. a resident assistant e. the hall director 7. How many times this semester have you talked about career information with: a. your academic adviser b. an instructor (professor, TA) c. a peer leader d. a resident assistant e. the hall director 119 8. How many times this semester have you talked about the ROSES seminar or program with: your academic adviser an instructor (professor, TA) a peer leader a resident assistant the hall director 9. How many times this semester have you talked about social concerns (i.e. roommate, friends, extracurricular activities) with: your academic adviser an instructor (professor, TA) a peer leader a resident assistant the hall director 10. How many time this semester have you talked about personal concerns (i.e. health, stresses) with: your academic adviser an instructor (professor, TA) a peer leader a resident assistant the hall director 1 1. How many times this semester have you talked about general concerns (your decision to come to MSU, campus resources) with: your academic adviser an instructor (professor, TA) a peer leader a resident assistant the hall director 120 ‘9' w . -'l' . i '\ 12. For each of the following items, please indicate your degree of satisfaction by placing the number from the scale in the space provided to the left of each item. Does not Very Very Apply Satisfied Satisfied Dissatisfied Dissatisfied O 1 2 3 4 5 a. your roommate relationship b. contact with an academic adviser c. contact with a peer leader 1” (1. contact with a resident assistant ‘ e. contact with an instructor I. 13. Please respond to the following items by placing the number from the scale in the space provided to the left of the item. " Not at all Somewhat Moderately well Fairly well Very well 1 2 3 4 5 a. How well do you feel your academic adviser knows you? b. How well do you feel your ROSES seminar instructor knows you? c. How well do you feel any of your instructors for other courses knows you? d. How well do you feel your resident assistant knows you? e. How well do you feel any of the ROSES peer leaders knows you? 14. How well do you believe the ROSES program has helped you in the following areas? Place your response from the scale in the space provided. Helped to a Helped to a Helped to a Helped Helped great extent good extent fair extent somewhat not at all 1 2 3 4 5 a Academically (i.e. classes, studying) b. Personally (i.e. support, assistance) c. Gain information about your major (I Gain information about MSU e Socially (i.e. meeting people, getting involved) 15. Would you recommend the ROSES program to another freshman in your major? Yes No 121 16. At this time, what is your intent regarding your major? (indicate the most likely choice by placing an “X” in the blank) __ stay in my current major __ change to a different major in science, engineering, or agriculture and natural resources __ change to a major outside of science, engineering, or agriculture and natural resources __ I have no idea 122 v __. x— A“_ _ ‘u . APPENDIX D APPENDIX D H0. VIP; h.m0. VIP.“ 00A 500 N00- v00- 00.0 m00 *40N0 **0N.0 2.3.0 420 .I.NN.0 ..sz0 ...L-m0 .10—.0 m00 :0N0 EEEom 50.0 00; 0~0- S0 :0 no.0 00.0 0.0- —0.0 M00 N00 2.0 m00- _00- 00.0 00.0- .2925 N00- 050- 00._ M00 N50 N50 00.0 50.0 _0.0 ELNO .420 N00 N00 500 :0 N00- >.Hmm .Eoou V0.0- 000 m00 00._ 0~0 S0 v0.0- 00.0- :0 000- V00 V00 V00 w00 1mN0 v00- \>> dam 00.0 :0 2.0 0—0 00; 20 **NNO 00.0- :0m0 1:0 :0 N00 500 ".3050 :VNO :0 .00m 30: mmmom m00 no.0 S0 00 N~ .0 00; 00.0- 0~ .0- 00.0 00.0 000- *0_ 0- 00.0- 0.0 2 .0 000- :5 amuam 1.0N0 00.0 00.0 v00- :NNO 000- 00.0 sum—.0 LNO v_0 *:.0 .420 ”INN. 2.0 00.0 .050 huam :0N0 S0- 500 000- 00.0- 0—0- .320 004 00.0 000 2.0 20 {-50 «*0N0 2.0 2.0 mmflU *20 0.0 _00 :0 :0m0 00.0 *shN0 00.0 00A .30 :VNO _0.0 :0N. 00.0 00.0 .4000 0&2“ 903 26911 42.0 m00 **_N.0 000- ::0 00.0 :0 00.0 “.30 004 2.0 2.3.0 20 4:0 .050 2.0 mMmOM :NNO N00 .420 #00 3.0 M00. ...:0 2.0 *svm0 2.0 00._ 20 ..2..mN.0 50.0 00.0 {L0 .>vd\3.umm _.:Lm0 20 N00 #00 N00 .40—.0- ...E0 20 0.0 *0~.0 2.0 00._ :0v0 0_ .0 @00- e0~0 #50 00.5 1590 m00- N00 V0.0 500 00.0- ......NNO *:.0 :0N0 2.0 ..imN0 .I.0v0 00A *smN0 m00- £4340 «aw—Eh 1.050 _0.0- 50.0 00.0 3.000 50.0 0~.0 **0N0 00.0 ...:0 50.0 0—0 $3.10 00.0 :50 ..imw0 =sm M00 00.0 2.0 **MNO :vm0 2.0 00.0 2.0 00.0 s0~0 00.0 V0.0- m00- .2100 00; 3.94.0 360m *e0N0 000- N00- v00- :0 000- .4050 2.0 *20 20 42.0 3,050 .3340 5300 334.0 00; owEovmodx mflmom é .Eoom .oom \3 fina< Bog .>0< <00 035 swvam mafia—U 30$ mmmom \3 0mm .020 =mm =3“— EEom fidu< 350830 98 85253 5.0 thE cone—250 -mm 2an 123 APPENDIX E \Q ....J OFFICE OF RESEARCH AND GRADUATE STUDIES University Commlttee on Research lnvolvln Human Subjects (UCRIHS) Michigan State University 246 Administration Building East Lansing, Michigan 48824-1046 517/355-2180 FAX: 517/432-1171 The M..:ntgan State University mashmmmmDme HMMWMAMM MSU 15 an ailments-action. coral-omortuntnr Institution APPENDIX E MICHIGAN STATE U N l V E R S l T Y October 24, 1997 TO: Kathryn M. Moore 418 Erickson Hall RE: IRB#: 97-513 TITLE: ADJUSTMENT TO COLLEGE: THE CONTRIBUTION OF A LIVING-LEARING PROGRAM FOR SCIENCE AND ENGINEERING STUDENTS REVISION REQUESTED: N/A CATEGORY: 1 - C APPROVAL DATE: 10/22/97 The University Committee on Research Involving Human Subjects'(UCRIHS) review of this project is complete._ I am pleased to adVise that the rights and welfare of the human subjects appear to be adequately rotected and methods to obtain informed consent are appropriate. herefore, above. RENEWAL : REVISIONS: PROBLEMS/ CHANGES: the UCRIHS approved this project and any reVisions listed UCRIHS approval is valid for one calendar year, beginning with the approval date shown above. Investigators planning to continue a project beyond one year must use the green renewal form (enclosed with t e original approval letter or when a project is renewed) to seek u date certification. There is a maXimum of four such expedite renewals possible. Investigators wishing to continue a preject beyond that time need to submit it again or complete reView. UCRIHS must review any changes in procedures involving human subjects, rior to initiation of t e change. If this is done at the time o renewal, please use the green renewal form. To revise an approved protocol at any 0 her time during the year, send your written request to the CRIHS Chair, requesting reVised approval and referencing the project's IRB # and title. Include in your request a description of the change and any revised instruments, consent forms or advertisements that are applicable. Should either of the following arise during the course of the work, investigators must noti y UCRIHS promptly: (l) roblems (unexpected Side effects, comp aints, etc.) involving uman subjects or (2) changes in the research environment or new information indicating greater risk to the human sub'ects than eXisted when the protocol was previously reviewed an approved. If we can be of any future help, please do not hesitate to contact us 2- at (517)355-2180 or FAX (517)4 Sincerely, DEW : bed vid E. Wright, CRIHS Chair 1171. cc: Cynthia K. Helman / ...—J I‘d Jo. APPENDIX F APPENDIX F October 6, 1997 Western Psychological Services Publishers and Distributors 12031 Wilshire Boulevard Los Angeles, CA 90025 A‘l 1N: Ms. Susan Weinberg I am a doctoral student in the Higher, Adult and Lifelong Education program at Michigan State University. The topic of my dissertation research is college student adjustment, and I would like to use the Student Adaptation to College Questionnaire. My research specifically examines the contribution of a living-learning program on the adjustment of first year science and engineering students. Several hundred first year students will receive the survey near the end of Fall Semester (December, 1997). In previous telephone communication with Western Psychological Services, I understand that there is a discount for purchasing instruments to be used in student research projects. Thus, I am requesting the discount for the Student Adaptation to College Questionnaire (hand-scored version). I will need the discount through May, 1998. This dissertation research is a requirement for the completion of my doctoral degree. In accordance with policies at Michigan State University, I am an enrolled student and will be enrolled during the time of data collection. All research procedures conform to APA standards as well as to the policies at Michigan State University governing research pr0jects. Sincerely, Cynthia K. Helman Doctoral Student Michigan State University (517)339-3632 email: helman@pilot.msu.edu 125 APPENDIX G APPENDIX C December ‘I, 1997 Dear ROSES student: I am a doctoral student in the Higher, Adult and Lifelong Education program here at MSU. I am conducting research for my dissertation and am requesting your assistance. The transition from high school and home to college and living in a residence hall is a period of adjustment for most students. My dissertation research focuses on the transition made by first semester students. In particular, I am interested in learning about the experiencesof students in the ROSES program and their transition to college. I am asking all students in the ROSES program this Fall to complete the enclosed two surveys. I know this is a busy time of the semester; however, I would appreciate it very much if you would take a few minutes to share your experiences with me by completing the surveys. The two surveys should take only about 25 minutes to complete. You may be assured of complete confidentiality. I will use your PlD only to match the two surveys, and to access selected institutional data (SAT/ACT score, high school GPA, predicted GPA, fall class schedule, and ROSES seminar attendance, fall semester GPA, and spring semester enrollment status). Once matched, all data will be assigned a random four digit number; date will be stored and analyzed only by that code. The code sheet linking Ple and the random numbers will be securely stored apart from all other data and records pertaining to this research project. Please return the completed surveys in a sealed envelope (enclosed) to the Bailey Hall Resident Director’s office (A101 Bailey) by December 8, 1997. You have had a variety of opportunities (i.e. attending an extra success seminar, attending a workshop, and other experiences announced by seminar instructors) to earn bonus points in the ROSES seminar. The ROSES seminar instructors have agreed that returning the survey by December 8, 1997 is also an opportunity to gain a bonus point toward your total ln-Class Activity Points for the ROSES seminar. When you turn in your surveys to the Resident Director, you will be asked to sign a slip of paper which will be fonuarded to your ROSES seminar instructor so you receive credit for returning the surveys. You indicate your voluntary agreement to participate by completing and returning the surveys. Participation in this study is voluntary and there is no penalty for not responding. You may choose to omit any question on the surveys. By providing your PID, you authorize me to access the institutional data indicated above. All results and information will be treated with strict confidence and all respondents will remain anonymous in all reports of the research findings. Copies of the results of this research will be available in Bailey Hall during the spring semester, 1998. I would be happy to answer any questions you may have about this study. I can be reached at 2- 2493 or helman@pilot.msu.edu. Thank you very much for your thoughtful responses and your assistance with this study. Cindy Helman Graduate Student 126 BIBLIOGRAPHY Van's-... ' Arr—1e.“ BIBLIOGRAPHY Astin, A. W. (1977). Preventing students from dropping out. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass, 1nc., Publishers. Atkinson, R. C. (1990). Supply and demand for scientists and engineers: A national crisis in the making. Science, 248, 425-432. Baker, R. W. (1986). The student adaptation to college questionnaire and its use ipan intervention study with freshmen. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the American College Personnel Association, New Orleans, LA. Baker, R. W., & Siryk, B. (1980). Alienation and freshman transition into college. Journal of College Student Personnelg19, 437-442. Baker, R. W., & Siryk, B. (1983). Social propensity and college adjustment. Journal of College Student Personnel, 24(4), 331-336. Baker, R. W., & Siryk, B. (1986). Exploratory intervention with a scale measuring adjustment to college. Journal of CougselinLPsychologL 33(1), 31-3 8. Baker, R. W., & Siryk, B. (1989). Student Adaptation to CollggaQaestionngirp (SACQ). Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services. Baker, R. W., Siryk, B., & McNeil, O. V. (1985). Expectation and reality in freshman adjustment to college. Jouml of Counseling Psychology, 32(1), 94-103. Barthelemy, K. J ., & Fine, M. A. (1995). The relations between residence hall climate and adjustment in college students. College Student Jomgk 29(4), 465-475. Blimling, G. S. (1993). The influence of college residence balls on students. In J. C. Smart (Ed), Higher education: Handbook of theory and research (Vol. D(, pp. 248- 307). New York: Agathon Press. Borg, W. R., & Gall, M. D. (1979). Edugagional research: An introduction. New York: Longrnan, Inc. Bragg, T. A. (1994). A study of the relationship between adiustrnent to college and freshman retention. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Oklahoma, Norman, Oklahoma. Brooks 11, J. H., & DuBois, D. L. (1995). Individual and environmental predictors of adjustment during the first year of college. Journal of College Student Development, 36(4), 347-360. 127 Buchanan, S. R. (1991). A longitudinal study of the effects of concurrently taking a modified university 10] course during the high school senior year on students’ anticipated adjustment to college and actual adjustment to college as compared to the adjustment to college of students who concurrently took a traditional lecture course or took no course during the high school senior year (Doctoral dissertation, University of South Carolina, 1991). Dissertation Abstracts IntemationLSZ-WA, 2386. Chickering, A. W. (1975). Commuting versus resident students. San Francisco: J ossey-Bass Publishers, Inc. Christie, N. G., & Dinham, S. M. (1991). Institutional and external influences on social integration in the freshman year. J ourn_aal of Higher Edugation. 62(4), 412-436. Cooper, S. E., & Robinson, D. A. G. (1988). Psychometric properties of the student adaptation to college questionnaire with engineering and science students. Measuremergaynd Evaluation in Counselingand Development, 21, 124-129. Cronbach, L. J. (1951). Coefficient alpha and the internal structure of tests. Psychometrika, 16(3), 297-334. Dahmus, S., Bemardin, H. J ., & Bemardin, K. (1992). Student adaptation to college questionnaire. Maasurement and Evaluation in Cougelingand Develgrment, 25, 139-142. Draper, R. W. (1991). On your own: The freshman experience at an American university (Doctoral dissertation, University of Oregon, 1991). Dissertation Abstra_g_t§ International 52-04A 1221. Feldman, K. A., & Newcomb, T. M. (1969). The impact of college on students. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass, Inc., Publishers. Fisher, S., & Hood, B. (1987). The stress of the transition to university: A longitudinal study of psychological disturbance, absent-mindedness and vulnerability to homesickness. British Journal of Psychology/J8, 425-441. George, D. & Mallery, P. (1995). SPSS/PC+ step by step: A simple guide gig reference. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth Publishing Company. Harris, B. C. (1991). The college student experience: How social support and loneliness relate to adaptation and attrition (Doctoral dissertation, The George Washington University, 1991). Dissertation Abstgrcts International 52-08B 4520. Jewler, A. J. (1989). Elements of an effective seminar: The university 101 program. In M. L. Upcrafi & J. N. Gardner (Eds), The freshman year experience: Helpinggtudents survive in collega . San Francisco: J ossey-Bass Publishers, Inc. 128 Johnson, G. M., Staton, A. Q., & Jorgensen-Earp, C. R. (1995). An ecological perspective on the transition of new university freshmen. Communication Education. 44, 336-352. Krotseng, M. V. (1992). Predicting persistence from the student adaptation to college questionnaire: Early warning or siren song? Reseaazh in Higher Education, 33(1), 99-111. Lapsley, D. K., Rice, K. G., & F itzGerald, D. P. (1990). Adolescent attachment, identity, and adj ustment to college: Implications for the continuity of adaptation hypothesis. Journal of Counseling and Development, 68, 561-565. Lapsley, D. K., Rice, K. G., & Shadid, G. E. (1989). Psychological separation and adjustment to college. Journal of Counseligg Psychology, 36(3), 286-294. Levine, A. (1989). Shaping higher education's future: Demographic glides anal opportunities, 1990-2000. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass Inc., Publishers. Lopez, F. G. (1991). Patterns of family conflict adn their relation to college student adjustment. Jourpal of Counselingand Develmgmerg69(3), 257-260. Lopez, F. 6., Campbell, V. L., & Watkins, C. E., Jr. (1988). Family structure, phsychological separation, and college adjustment: A canonical analysis and cross- validation. Journal of Counselinngchology, 35(4), 402-409. Lopez, F. G., Campbell, V. L., & Watkins, C. E., Jr. (1986). Depression, psychological separation, and college adjustment: An investigation of sex differences. Journal of Counaeling Psychology, 33, 53-56. Martin, N. K., & Dixon, P. N. (1989). The effects of freshman orientation and locus of control on adjustment to college. Journal of Coflgge Student Development, 3_0(4), 362-367. Mooney, S. P., Sherman, M. F., & LoPresto, C. T. (1991). Academic locus of control, self-esteem, and perceived distance from home as predictors of college adjustment. Journal of Counseling and Development, 69(5), 445-448. Murguia, E., Padilla, R. V., & Pavel, M. (1991). Ethnicity and the concept of social integration in Tinto's model of institutional departure. Journal of College Student Development, 32(5), 433-439. Nelson, R. B., Scott, T. B., & Bryan, W. A. (1984). Precollege characteristics and early college experiences as predictors of freshman year persistence. Journal of College Student Personnel, 25(1), 50-54. 129 Noel, L. (1985). Increasing student retention: new challenges and potential. In L. Noel, R. Levitz, D. Saluri, & Associates (Eds), Increasiag student retention: Effective progzams and prrgices for reducingghe dropout r_a_ta (pp. 1-27). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass, Inc., Publishers. Nosow, S. (1975). An attitudinal comparison of residential college seniors with other seniors. Journal of College Student Personnel, 16(1), 17-23. Panos, R. J ., & Astin, A. W. (1968). Attrition among college students. American Educatirml Research Journal, 5(1), 57-72. Pascarella, E. T., & Terenzini, P. T. (1980). Predicting freshman persistence and voluntary dropout decisions from a theoretical model. Journal of Higher Edugtion. 51 (1), 60-75. Pascarella, E. T., & Terenzini, P. T. (1991). How college affects students. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass Inc., Publishers. Pascarella, E. T., Terenzini, P. T., & Wolfle, L. M. (1986). Orientation to college and freshman year persistence/withdrawal decisions. Journal of Higher Education. 57(2), 15 5-1 75. Pemberton, C. ( 1969). An Evaluation of a Living-Learning Residence Ha1_l Progzam (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. ED 077 082) Pool, R. (1990). Who will do science in the 19903? Science, 248, 433-435. Rice, K. G. (1992). Separation-individuation and adjustment to college: A longitudinal study. Journal of Cou_aspling Psychology. 39(2), 203-213. Seymour E. (1992). Undergraduate problems with teaching and advising in SME majors—Explaining gender differences in attrition rates. Journal of College Science Teaching, 21(5), 284-292. Schwitzer, A. M., McGovern, T. V., & Robbins, S. B. (1991). Adjustment outcomes of a freshman seminar: A utilization-focused approach. Journal of Collega Student Development, 32(6), 484-489. Smith, M. A., & Baker, R. W. (1987). Freshman decidedness regarding academic major and adjustment to college. Psychological Reports. 61. 847-853. Spady, W. G. (1970). Dropouts from higher education: An interdisciplinary review and synthesis. Interchange, 1(1), 64-85. Spady, W. G. (1971). Dropouts from higher education: Toward an empirical model. Interchange, 2(3), 38-62. 130 Stoecker, J ., Pascarella, E. T., & Wolfle, L. M. (1988). Persistence in higher education: A 9-year test of a theoretical model. Journal of College Student Development, 29(3), 196-209. Terenzini, P. T., Allison, K. W., Millar, S. B., Rendon, L. 1., Upcraft, M. L., Gregg, P., & Jalomo, R. (1992). The transition to college project: Final report . University Park, PA: The Pennsylvania State University. Terenzini, P. T., & Pascarella, E. T. (1976). The relation of freshman students' social and academic integration munition (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. ED 126 843 ) Terenzini, P. T., & Pascarella, E. T. (1977). Voluntary freshman attrition and patterns of social and academic integration in a university: A test of a conceptual model. Researph in Higher Education, 6, 25-43. Terenzini, P. T., & Pascarella, E. T. (1978). The relation of students' precollege characteristics and freshman year experience to voluntary attrition. Research in Higher Education, 9, 347-366. Terenzini, P. T., Rendon, L. I., Upcraft, M. L., Millar, S. B., Allison, K. W., Gregg, P. L., & Jalomo, R. (1996). The transition to college: Diverse students, diverse stories. In F. K. Stage, G. L. Anaya, J. P. Bean, D. Hossler, & G. D. Kuh (Eds), College students: The evolving nature of research (pp. 54-65). Needham Heights, MA: Simon and Schuster Custom Publishing. Tinto, V. (1975). Dropout from higher education: A theoretical synthesis of recent research. Review of Educational Research, 45(1), 89-125. Tinto, V. (1985). Dropping out and other forms of withdrawal from college. In L. Noel, R. Levitz, D. Saluri, & and Associates (Eds), Increasing student retention: Effective pr_ogams and praatices for reducingahe dropout rata (pp. 28—43). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass, Inc., Publishers. Tinto, V. (1988). Stages of student departure: Reflections on the longitudinal character of student leaving. Jourpal of Higgr Education, 59(4), 438-455. Tinto, V. (1993). Leaving college: rethinking the causes and cures of student attrition. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press. Upcraft, M. L., & Gardner, J. N. (1989). The freshman year experience: Helping student survive in collega. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass Publishers, Inc. Van Gennep, A. (1960). The rites of passage (M. B. Vizedom & G. L. Caffee, Trans). Chicago: The University of Chicago Press. 131 Wallace, W. L. (1966). Student culture: Sociaj structure and continuity in a liberal arts college. Chicago: Aldine Publishing Co. Wilson, R. J ., Anderson, S. A., & Fleming, W. M. (1987). Commuter and resident students' personal and family adjustment. Journal of Collage Student Personnel, _2_§(3), 229-233. Zeller, W. J ., & Mosier, R. (1993). Culture shock and the first year experience. The Journal of College and University Student Housing. 23(2), 19-23. Zmich, R., Lux, P., DeRosa, S., & Gordon, T. (1997). Student guide to the ROSES experience (5th Anniversary) [Brochure]. East Lansing, MI: Michigan State University. 132 I'IICHIGRN STATE UNIV. LIBRARIES lllllllllllllllllllllllllllllllllllllllllllllllllllllllllllll 31293018342067