1!. mmé" ' hfivwwséwm $3., mm, a.» , I nrvgJ: .— 4 , mgfifin 1%? a A fiamfi, .. Ed». . w». amwaf ,m . r .t .i 4.5)... ht 7 .. r:- spanking“. . £511.13: .5 .. ... . t. «355 a A iJn: .514”. 2&13fll, . >95! 3“”... . .2} . :83... 1:: is. 141. Inll . 1:. . khan“ a. e}... .. r. 57:... ’51-“?! ,. .I , . 3. r ..l haunt!“ . a :.wu...t[zht.c.l 5.1: {I}, I?! :33}. 12§x.xn . 15.5.! A . , ”ED-M“; I! , :ll‘! .. J .v. in“! ‘l’ , I :‘l i.hflflvpl’fluh..ml.u.hll." .. 35f . )tkfl 11.6.2.3: “VA. 3.3:..5. ) D. » .fslvincpsi. 5.71.; ((1,: . .z—ékbflfi: .1:...otl.!031.: .2 III. . 3.1 .5555: : nil"?! . . .II g...ii...¢!t it! x). ‘5‘... . :11 35.1.2.1. . . 3. Pl! .nfirl. , 51hr in: , 3 If)...” at: .ooLHPZ} fiwwnfi. mréuxrrmfifim : ‘ ‘ . . _ . ., _ . , . . . .gwt ”mi." .uowrwwv. , . ,A . . V E llfliflfimflmfliflfifliflflfliflifit ‘2’: «CU LIBRARY r Michigan State University This is to certify that the thesis entitled A CROSS-CULTURAL COMPARISON OF AGGRESSIVE COMMUNICATION TRAITS; VERBAL AGGRESSIVENESS, ARGUMENTATIVENESS, AND ASSERTIVENESS presented by Sachiyo Morinaga has been accepted towards fulfilIment of the requirements for MA degree in Nmmcm ION .. 2&6 Major professor Date flZfi/W-é W 0-7639 MS U is an Affirmative Action/Equal Opportunity Institution PLACE IN RETURN BOX to remove this checkout from your record. To AVOID FINES return on or before date due. MAY BE RECALLED with earlier due date if requested. DATE DUE DATE DUE DATE DUE mi 2 9 4W1 mm m.“ A CROSS-CULTURAL COMPARISON OF AGGRESSIVE COMMUNICATION TRAITS; VERBAL AGGRESSIVENESS, ARGUMENTATIVENESS, AND ASSERTIVENESS. BY Sachiyo Morinaga A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF ARTS Department of Communication 2000 PENTPRACT A CROSS-CULTURAL COMPARISON OF AGGRESSIVE COMMUNICATION TRAITS; VERBAL AGGRESSIVENESS, ARGUMENTATIVENESS, AND ASSERTIVENESS. BY Sachiyo Morinaga This study compares aggressive communication traits in three countries, China, Japan and the United States. Aggressive communication traits can be distinguished into two categories; constructive traits (i.e. assertiveness, argumentativeness) and destructive traits (i.e. hostility, verbal aggressiveness). This study analyzed tendencies of these communication traits displayed among participants in three countries, employing the Argumentativeness Scale (Infante & Rancer, 1982), the Verbal Aggressiveness Scale (Infante & Wigley, 1986), and the Assertiveness Scale (Richmond & McCroskey, 1992). The results indicated significant interaction effects of country and sex with the main effects of both on all three communication traits. In addition, implications and limitations of these findings as well as suggestions for future research are discussed. Copy Right Sachiyo Morinaga 2000 To My Parents; Teru and Satoshi, Morinaga ACKNOWLEDGEMENT First and foremost, I would like to thank Dr. William Donohue, Dr. Mary Bresnahan and Dr. William Mehrens for your supports and suggestions in completing my thesis. I especially would like to appreciate Dr. Bresnahan for her sharing data from her research team. I would like to thank Rie and Lifang for their precious comments and warm friendship. I thank the Rotary International for allowing me this great opportunity to study communication at Michigan State University. Also, I thank officials and members of MSU Japan Club, and all the teachers at Battle Creek Japanese Saturday School. Lastly but definitely not least, I want to thank my friends who supported me during past few years. Special thanks go to David, Keiko, Jerry, Cousins-sensei, Kimika, Sawa, Hiromi, Yuuka, Okada-san, the Inoue family, the Shearman, Lihua, Jaemin, and Sunyoung. I can not list all the names here, but without your warm friendship, I would not have been able to enjoy studying here as much as I had. TABLE OF CONTENTS List of Tables List of Figures List of Abbreviations Chapter1:Introduction ChapterZ: Culture Overview Definitions of Culture Cultural Variability Individualism-Collectivism Cultures Selected Chapter3: Aggressive Communication Verbal Aggressiveness Verbal Aggressiveness and Culture Verbal Aggressiveness and Sex Argumentativeness Argumentativeness and Culture Argumentativeness and Sex Assertiveness Assertiveness and Culture Assertiveness and Sex Chapter4: Research Questions Chapters: Methods Participants Procedures Translations Instrumentation The Verbal Aggressiveness Scale The Argumentativeness Scale The Assertiveness Scale Preliminary Analyses Chapter6: Results Scale Intercorrelations Descriptive Statistics for four scales Two-way ANOVA results country by sex The Verbal Aggressiveness Scale T-test results of each country pair The ARGap Scale T—test results of each country pair w viii 31 32 32 33 33 34 34 34 35 36 40 40 41 42 42 42 43 43 The ARGav Scale T-test results of each country pair The Assertiveness Scale T-test results of each country pair Comparison by Sex T-tests between country T—tests within country Chapter 7: Discussion Limitations References Appendices Appendix A Tables Appendix B Figures Appendix C Questionnaires Research Consent Form vii 43 44 44 45 45 45 46 47 5.2 57 67 68 69 84 85 87 88 93 List of Tables Table 1: Correlations and errors of the Verbal Aggressiveness Scale items left after the tests of internal consistency Table 2: Correlations and errors of the ARGap Scale left after the tests of internal consistency Table 3: Correlations and errors of the ARGav Scale items left after the tests of internal consistency Table 4: Correlations and errors of the Assertiveness Scale items left after the tests of internal consistency Table 5: Scale Intercorrelations of four scales Table 6: Descriptive Statistics (Mean, Standard Deviation, and Sample Size) of the Verbal Aggressiveness Scale Table 7: Descriptive Statistics (Mean, Standard Deviation, and Sample Size) of the ARGap Scale Table 8: Descriptive Statistics (Mean, Standard Deviation, and Sample Size) of the ARGav Scale Table 9: Descriptive Statistics (Mean, Standard Deviation, and Sample Size) of the Assertiveness Scale Table 10: Two-way ANOVA results of the Verbal Aggressiveness Scale country by sex Table 11: T-test result of the Verbal Aggressiveness Scale comparing three pairs of countries Table 12: Two-way ANOVA results of the ARGap Scale country by sex Table 13: T-tests result of the ARGap Scale comparing three pairs of countries Table 14: Two-way ANOVA results of the ARGav Scale country by sex Table 15: T-tests result of the ARGav Scale comparing three pairs of countries viii Table 16: Two-way ANOVA results of the Assertiveness country by sex Table 17: T-tests result of the Assertiveness Scale comparing three pairs of countries Table 18: T-test results of four scales female across three countries Table 19: T-test results of four scales female in the United States Table 20: T-test results of four scales female in Japan Table 21: T-test results of four scales female in China ix between male between male between male between male Scale and and and and Figure 1: Line graph Aggressiveness Scale Figure 2: Line graph country and sex Figure 3: Line graph country and sex Figure 4: Line graph Scale by country and List of Figures of mean scores by country and of mean scores of mean scores of mean scores sex for the Verbal sex for the ARGap Scale by for the ARGav Scale by for the Assertiveness List of Abbreviations ARGap: argumentativeness approach arguments) ARGav: argumentativeness arguments) ARth: argumentativeness arugmentative) CFA: confirmatory factor Df: Degree of freedom N: Sample Size Std. Dev.: Sig.: Significance level approache (the tendency to avoidance (the tendency to avoid general (the general trait to be analysis Standard Deviation m Chapter 1 Introduction This study investigates cultural differences in aggressive communication traits. Aggressive communication traits can be classified into two kinds: destructive traits and constructive traits (Infante and Rancer, 1996). Destructive traits include verbal aggressiveness and hostility. Constructive traits refer to argumentativeness and assertiveness. This study examines three of these traits, verbal aggressiveness, argumentativeness and assertiveness. Three countries (i.e. China, Japan, and the United States) were chosen for consideration for this study. Numerous studies have examined communication behaviors in terms of individual predispositions or traits. Infante, Rancer, and Womack (1990) described communication traits as hypothetical constructs that explain “enduring consistencies and differences in individuals’ message-sending and message- receiving behaviors”(p. 143). Also, according to Martin and Anderson (1996), “communication traits are subsets of personality traits concerned with human symbolic behavior” (p.58). This study investigates whether cultural and sex differences exist in three communication traits; verbal l aggressiveness, argumentativeness, and assertiveness. These communication traits were selected for the present study for several reasons. First, they have educational implications, such as training and second language instruction. Argumentativeness and assertiveness are described as elements of communication competence and considered to be desirable communication traits, whereas verbal aggressiveness represents one of the undesirable communication traits (Infante, 1987; McCroskey & Richmond, 1992). The distinction between argumentativeness and verbal aggressiveness has been emphasized by many scholars. Argumentativeness often leads to positive outcomes, such as obtaining goals, keeping self-esteem, better understanding, and relational development; whereas verbal aggressiveness often leads to negative outcomes. However, much of the research examining aggressive communication traits that has been conducted in the U. S. might not hold true for people from other cultures. Therefore, in communicating cross- culturally, it is crucial to understand the general communication traits predominantly displayed by people in other cultures. A second reason for the importance of studying aggressive communication is to understand how people from 2 different cultures deal with conflictual situations that often result in arguing. Now that it is increasingly important for most of the countries on the earth to communicate with each other, it is beneficial for us to know what types of communicative traits are displayed in different countries. Folger, Poole, and Stutman (1993) discussed that argumentativeness and verbal aggressiveness were important predisposional traits that affected the processes and outcomes of interpersonal conflicts (pp.51- 51). Therefore, exploration of verbal aggressiveness and argumentativeness should provide insight for the study of conflict across cultures. Chapter 2 Culture Overview E E' 'l' E : 1 “Culture” connotes large and profound meanings, which complicates the task of definition. There have been many attempts to examine culture. Among the prevalent definitions of culture, intercultural communication has been influenced mostly by definitions proposed by anthropologists and psychologists. For example, Clifford Geertz(1973) defined culture as “an historically transmitted pattern of meaning embodied in symbols, a system of inherited conceptions expressed in symbolic forms by means of which men communicate, perpetuate and develop their knowledge about and attitudes toward life”(p.89). A renowned social psychologist, Geert Hofstede(1980) defined culture similarly, when he defined it as the “programming of the mind” and as the “integrative aggregate of common characteristics that influence a human group’s response to its environment” (Hofstede, 1980, p.21). In the communication field, Samover, Porter, and Jain (1994) defined culture as “the deposit of knowledge, experience, beliefs, values, attitudes, meanings, hierarchies, religion, 4 notions of time, roles, spatial relations, concepts of the universe, and material objects and possessions acquired by a group of people in the course of generations through individual and group striving” (p.11). With this plethora of definitions of culture, all seem to agree on the notion that culture explains shared behaviors, values or assumptions of a specific group of people. Thus, it can be said one of the factors that makes us behave the way we do is this shared assumption of the group or the culture. The cultural literature claims that people in different cultures behave differently because they have divergent beliefs. Several studies show that culture explains the differences of behavior among different cultures. There is ample justification for assuming that an individual’s predisposition to act in a predictable way in an argumentative situation may be strongly influenced by culture. Based on this notion, the present study examines whether cultural factors can be one way of explaining communicative traits, verbal aggressiveness, argumentativeness, and assertiveness. : J! J M . 1.]. Many intercultural literatures have the basic assumption that individual’s behaviors are affected by 5 culture. However, general cultural influence is so vague and difficult to pin down. Efforts to narrow it down and identify cultural variability have been made by several scholars. Hofstede (1980) did very extensive research on cultural variability displayed in the multinational corporation. Hofstede (1980) and other scholars studied organizational behaviors in IBM subsidiaries in 53 countries around the world and identified several dimensions and variables of culture. Many studies have been conducted on the dimensions of cultural variability that Hofstede introduced. These dimensions include Power Distance, Uncertainty Avoidance, Masculinity—Femininity, and Individualism-Collectivism. Power distance refers to “the extent to which the members of a society accept that power in institutions and organizations is distributed unequally”(Hofstede, 1983, p. 336). This dimension deals with the perception of equality and power. Uncertainty avoidance is defined as “the degree to which the members of a society feel uncomfortable with uncertainty and ambiguity, which leads them to support beliefs promising certainty and to maintain institutions protecting conformity”(Hofstede, 1983, p.336). This dimension deals with how people in a culture react to uncertainty. Masculinity represents “a 6 preference for achievement, heroism, assertiveness and material success", while femininity represents “a preference for relationship, modesty, caring for the weak, and the quality of life”(Hofstede, 1983, p. 337). Masculinity and femininity deal with the different values that tend to predominate in a culture. Lastly, Hofstede (1983) described individualism as a “loosely knit social framework in society in which individuals are supposed to take care of themselves and their immediate families only”, in contrast with collectivism, which is illustrated as a “tightly knit social framework in which individuals can expect their relatives, clan or other in—group to look after them, in exchange for unquestioning loyalty”(p.337). This dimension deals with the relationships of people in a culture. The present study particularly focuses on Individualism-Collectivism, for this dimension has been actively used for intercultural communication research. Individualism-Collectivism defines the opposing anchor points of a continuum of values along which cultures are arrayed. Individualism-Collectivism is a widely accepted dimension which has been studied both conceptually and empirically (Hofstede, 1980; Hui, 1988; Triandis, 1995; 7 Triandis, Bontempo, Villareal, Asai, Lucca, 1988; Triandis, McCusker, Hui, 1990). In individualistic cultures, individual goals are emphasized more than group goals. In comparison, in collectivistic cultures, group goals are valued more than goals of individuals. Individualistic cultures tend to focus more on individual initiative and achievement, while collectivistic cultures tend to put emphasis on inclusion and group harmony. The Individualism-Collectivism dimension can also be illustrated well with the notion of “ingroup”. Triandis et al. (1988) defined ingroup as follows, “ingroups are groups of people about whose welfare one is concerned, with whom one is willing to cooperate without demanding equitable returns and separation from whom leads to discomfort or even pain” (p.75). People in individualistic cultures tend to have several ingroups to which they belong to, such as family, social clubs, work places, schools, religious groups and so on, while members of collectivistic cultures tend to belong to fewer general ingroups. Also, ingroups have less influence in individualistic cultures, while they have strong influence on activities in collectivistic cultures. Individualistic cultures tend to be universalistic and apply 8 the same value standard to all situations, while collectivistic cultures tend to be particularistic and apply different value standards depending on the groups, ingroup and outgroup (Gudykunst & Matsumoto, 1996). Triandis (1995) further explained Individualism- Collectivism by describing the power relationships that people in each culture have. Triandis (1995) termed these as “vertical culture” and “horizontal culture”. In vertical culture people are expected to stand out from others, and tend to see themselves as different from others, while those who are in a horizontal culture tend to see themselves the same as others, valuing conformity to groups and devaluing individual freedom. One good example of this is illustrated by contrasting proverbs from each country. In Japan, a proverb says, “the nail that sticks out will get hammered (Deru Kui Wa Utareru)”. This proverb teaches Japanese not to stand out as an individual and instead emphasized the importance of being one of a coordinated member. Also, in Chinese, a proverb says “the roof support beam that sticks out most rots first (Mao Jian De Suen Zi Xian Lau)". This proverb tells the Chinese not to be the first one who stands out or speaks out. By contrast, in the U.S., a proverb says, “the squeaky wheel gets the grease”. This proverb teaches 9 Americans that calling attention to yourself is a good strategy to get some form of rewards. Earlier on, Hall (1976) identified the difference between high-context communication and low—context communication. Hall (1976) explained high-low context communication as follows. A high context communication or message is one in which “most of the information is either in the physical context or internalized in the person, while very little is in the coded, explicit, transmitted part of the message”(p76). On the other hand, a low-context communication or message is one in which the “mass of information is vested in the explicit code”(p. 70). Several studies revealed the relationship of High-Low Context communication and the Individualism—Collectivism Scale. Gudykunst and Matsumoto (1996) claimed that “high and low context communication are a function of individualism- collectivism“(p.33). Gudykunst & Matsumoto (1996) argued that both individualism and collectivism coexist in all cultures, but either one tends to predominate. For example, cultures that tend to be individualistic include Australia, Belgium, Canada, Denmark, France, Germany, Great Britain, Ireland, Israel, Italy, the Netherlands, New Zealand, Norway, Sweden, 10 Switzerland, and the United States. Cultures that tend to be collectivistic include Brazil, China, Colombia, Egypt, Greece, India, Japan, Kenya, Korea, Mexico, Nigeria, Pakistan, Panama, Peru, Saudi Arabia, Thailand, Venezuela, and Vietnam (Gudykunst & Matsumoto, 1996, p. 24). CLAW In this study, three countries, the People’s Republic of China, Japan, and the United States were chosen for consideration, because they contrast greatly in their communication behavior. These three countries differ in several cultural dimensions, such as collectivism- individualism (Hofstede, 1980; Triandis et al., 1988), and high—low context communication (Hall, 1976). Furthermore, the United States is considered as representative of individualism, while China and Japan are considered as representative of collectivism. According to the scores displayed by Gudykunst and Ting-Toomey (1988), Japan scored 46 on individualism and can be considered as a high-context communication culture. The United States scored 91 on individualism scale and can be considered as a low-context communication culture. Unfortunately, because of the political condition with the People's Republic of China, they did not have scores of 11 China. They, however, presented scores for Taiwan, Hong Kong, and Singapore. All three cultures showed very low individualism scores; Taiwan scored 17, Hong Kong scored 25, and Singapore scored 20 on individualism scale. Even though these three countries have many Chinese immigrants and share the same historical background, it might be problematic for us to assume that these three countries would display similar orientation to individualism. Several studies have examined whether Individualism- Collectivism is a function of divergent communication behaviors. For example, Kim (1994) confirmed that people from collectivistic cultures are more concerned about hurting others and making requests from others, while people from individualistic cultures are concerned more about speaking clearly in conversation. Also, Kim and Wilson (1994) observed that people in individualistic cultures tend to regard clarity as the essential element of effective communication in comparison to people from collectivistic cultures. Overall, culture seems to reinforce different values in different cultures, and accordingly makes people behave different ways. Because collectivistic cultures reinforce concern for group harmony, while individualistic cultures 12 reinforce concern for individual goals, the divergent attitudes and communication behaviors will predictably be displayed in an arguing situation. Therefore, this study attempts to compare the differences in aggressive communication traits of individuals from an individualistic country (i.e. the U.S.) with collectivistic countries (i.e. Japan and China). 13 Chapter 3 Aggressive Communication Infante and Rancer (1996) claimed that “aggression can be good or bad or both good and bad” and distinguished aggressive communication traits and divided into two categories; constructive aggression and destructive aggression (p.322). The constructive traits include assertiveness and argumentativeness, while destructive communication traits include verbal aggressiveness and hostility. Argumentativeness and assertiveness are seen as constructive communication traits. According to Infante and Rancer (1996), assertiveness is the more global trait and argumentativeness is “a subset of assertiveness because all argument is assertive, but not all assertiveness involves argument” (p.322). A more formal definition of each trait will be provided later in this text, but in general, the reader should be aware that assertiveness deals with being forthright about your needs and worth, while argumentativeness deals with offering reasons to support your position. Conversely, verbal aggressiveness and hostility are 14 considered as destructive traits of aggressive communication and are viewed “as a subset of a more global trait, in this case, hostility”, because “all verbal aggression is hostile, but not all hostility involves verbal aggression” (Infante & Rancer, 1996. P.323). Hostility is the general mode of being hostile to others, while verbal aggressiveness entails personal attacks on others verbally. The distinction between constructive aggression and destructive aggression has been often emphasized, especially, verbal aggressiveness and argumentativeness both conceptually (Infante & Rancer, 1996; Infante, Rancer,& Womack, 1993) and empirically (Infante & Rancer, 1982; Infante & Wigley, 1986). This distinction is important also because argumentativeness often leads to positive outcomes, such as obtaining goals, retaining self—esteem, better understanding and relational development, while verbal aggressiveness often leads to negative outcomes. Wee Verbal aggressiveness refers to one’s tendency to derogate another person, or attack the self-esteem of another, while argumentativeness refers to one's tendency to provide logic and reasoning to support one's position on controversial issues. Infante and Wigley (1986) described 15 verbal aggressiveness as “attacking an individual’s self- concept in order to make a person feel less favorably about self”(p.61). Verbal aggressiveness is differentiated from argumentativeness in the focus of the attack; verbal aggressiveness deals with attacking the self-concept of others, while argumentativeness deals with attacking the opinions of others. Several studies support the idea that verbal aggression is a self-concept attack directed toward another’s self-concept (Geen & Donnerstein, 1983; Roloff & Greenberg, 1979). Verbally aggressive messages include “character attacks, competence attacks, insults, maledictions, teasing, ridicule, profanity, and nonverbal emblems”(p.61). According to Kinney (1994), three broad domains of self-concept attack are presented; group membership, personal failings, and relational failings. Kinney (1994) also identified four attributions of verbal aggression; psychopathology, disdain, social learning and argumentative skill deficiencies. First, psychopathology causes verbal aggression in that some people try to solve problems by being verbally aggressive with others who they identify their problem. Second, disdain causes verbal aggression when by looking down on others motivates one to be verbally aggressive. Third, social learning can also be 16 the cause for one’s verbal aggression, since the environment can reinforce one to use verbally aggressive messages. Bayer and Cegala (1992) examined the relationships between trait verbal aggressiveness and argumentativeness with parenting style. They found that verbal aggression correlates positively with control—hostile parenting style. Fourth, argumentative skill deficiencies, or lack of the ability to argue causes some to be verbally aggressive. Infante et a1. (1987) concluded that the verbal aggression is partly a result of a lack of argumentative skill, since they found that a highly argumentative person was the least provoked to appeal to verbal aggression. In addition, frustration, in which one is discouraged or hindered from doing something, was considered as one reason for verbal aggression (Infante, 1987). ygIhal_Aggressiyeness_and_gultu1§+ Several cross-cultural studies are available on conflict management and assertiveness, but less on verbal aggressiveness and argumentativeness. Harman, Klopf, and Ishii (1990) examined differences in verbal aggressiveness between Japanese and American college students. They found no significant overall differences between Japanese and Americans, Japanese females and American Females, and Japanese males and American males, 17 but they found a significant difference between males and females collapsing the country factor. Sueda and Wiseman (1992) examined how Americans and Japanese differ in dealing with embarrassment. Sueda and Wiseman (1992) found that Americans used so called, “autonomy-preserving strategies” such as mere presentation of the fact, justification, humor, and aggression significantly more often than Japanese did in dealing with embarrassment. This finding is consistent with the former studies that concluded individuals in low-context cultures tended to use more autonomy-preserving strategies than individuals in high-context cultures (Ting-Toomey, 1988). In a cross-ethnicity study, which was conducted with co-cultural groups in the U.S., Sanders, Gass, Wiseman and Bruschke (1992) examined the three argumentation trait’s measures; verbal aggression, argumentativeness and need for cognition. Sanders et al. (1992) found that argumentativeness will correlate positively with a need for cognition, while verbal aggressiveness will correlate negatively with a need for cognition. Sanders et al. (1992) also examined the relationship between an individuals’ ethnicity and verbal aggressiveness as well as argumentativeness. They found Asian Americans to be significantly higher on the verbal aggressiveness scale than 18 either European Americans or Hispanics, although they found no significant differences on the argumentativeness scale among Asian Americans, European Americans and Hispanics, contrary to other findings that concluded Asian Americans tended to be more indirect or assertive than Caucasians (Sue & Sue, 1977; Zane, Sue, Hu and Kwon, 1991). Although there are some studies that concluded culture explains their choice of conflict strategies, there is no evidence supporting that culture explains an individual’s trait to be verbally aggressive. Therefore, the following hypothesis is posited. H1: There will be no significant difference in verbal aggressiveness among three countries; China, Japan, and the United Status. yerbal_Aggressiveness_and_fiex* Several studies demonstrate the sex differences in verbal aggressiveness concluding that males tend to be more verbally aggressive than females (Infante et al., 1984; Infante & Wigley, 1986; Nicotera & Rancer, 1994). Infante (1989) examined whether males and females responded differently to highly argumentative others. Infante (1989) observed that males responded with more verbal aggression when others used verbal aggression, whereas females became more argumentative. Harman, Klopf, 19 and Ishii (1990) found significant sex differences in verbal aggressiveness among Japanese and American subjects collapsing the country factor. Several explanations have been offered to describe the sex differences in verbal aggressiveness. One of the explanations is that males and females differ in their perception of verbal aggressiveness and argumentativeness. Nicotera and Rancer (1994) reported significant differences between the sexes on both self-reported and stereotyped perceptions of verbal aggressiveness as well as argumentativeness. Another explanation is that there is sex difference in social expectations. It is said that males are expected to be more aggressive than females (Burgoon & Miller, 1983). Based on these observations, the following hypothesis is warranted. H2: Males in all three countries can be expected to exhibit more verbal aggressiveness than females counterparts. Argumentatiitensss According to Infante and Rancer (1982), Argumentativeness is defined as “a generally stable trait that predisposes the individual in communication situations to advance positions on controversial issues and to attack verbally the positions that other people take on these 20 issues” (p. 72). As argued in the verbal aggressiveness section, argumentativeness is differentiated from verbal aggressiveness in that they differ in the focus of attack. Namely, argumentativeness refers to an individual’s tendency to argue on controversial issues, while verbal aggressiveness refers to the tendency to attack another person’s self-esteem. Argumentativeness is conceptualized in terms of approach—avoidance, or excitation-inhibition of conflict. The highly argumentative person feels excitement toward argument and has a strong tendency to approach arguments, without feeling inhibition to argue or avoiding arguments. The low argumentative person has a strong tendency to avoid arguments and shows no favorable excitement with strong inhibitions and avoidance tendencies (Infante & Rancer, 1982). Therefore, the general trait to be argumentative (Hereafter abbreviated as ARth) is constituted by two factors; tendency to approach argument (Hereafter abbreviated as ARGap) and tendency to avoid arguments (Hereafter abbreviated as ARGav). The general trait for argumentativeness was hypothesized as the difference of sum of ARGap minus ARGav. Hence, ARth = ARGap - ARGav. 21 The highly argumentative person is high in an argumentative approach and low in avoidance, while a low argumentative individual would be low in an argumentative approach and high in avoidance. In case the score for approach and avoidance is close in number, a person will have moderate score in general argumentativeness. This person is classified as a moderate, and divided into two types of moderates; conflict feeling moderates and apathetic moderates. Conflict feeling moderates are high on both approach and avoidance. They want to argue, but are reluctant to do so at the same time. Apathetic moderates are low in both approach and avoidance. They do not want to engage in arguments, though they are not intentionally avoiding argument. However, some evidences suggests that the Argumentativeness Scale is in fact unidimensional (Boster & Lavubnem 1988; Kazoleas, 1993; Roberto & Finucase, 1997). If the Argumantativeness Scale is not actually unidimentional, it is not appropriate for us to substruct those scores. Therefore, the present study conducts analyses for both a unidimensional solution and a multidimensional solution. WW Several studies have related Individualism—Collectivism to a conflict style (Ting-Toomey, 22 1988, 1991; Ting-Toomey, Gao, Trubiskey, Yang, Kim, Lin, & Nishida, 1991). Ting-Toomey and associates consistently found that people in collectivistic cultures tend to use avoidance strategy more often than people in individualistic cultures. Prunty, Klopf, and Ishii (1990a, 1990b) examined differences 1J1 argumentativeness between Japanese and American college students. They found that Americans were significantly higher on both tendencies to approach arguments and the general trait to be argumantative. In conflict management, Ohbushi and Takahashi (1994) studied the cross-cultural comparison. of conflict style and its motivations for the strategies chosen. They found that Japanese tend to avoid conflict more than Americans and Japanese avoidance was motivated. by their' perception. of shared responsibility and desire to maintain relationships. Similarly, Sueda and Wiseman (1992) found that Americans used. so called, “autonomy-preserving strategies” such. as mere presentation of facts, justification, humor, and aggression significantly more than Japanese did in dealing with embarrassment. In Japan, having no excuse for one's failure, or being able to surrender is often considered as a virtue, because it often leads us to peaceful interpersonal relationships. Morris et a1. (1998) examined conflict 23 management style of managers in the U.S., China, Philippines, and India and concluded that Chinese managers rely more on an avoiding style, whereas American managers rely more on a competing style. A review of several studies supported the evidence regarding the differences in argumentative communication traits across cultures. Therefore, following hypothesis is warranted. H3: There will be significant difference in the argumentativeness scale among three countries; China, Japan, and the United Status, Americans being more argumentative than Japanese and Chinese. Argumentatixeness_and_3ex‘ Numerous scholars have found gender differences in argumentativeness (Infante, 1982; Nicotera & Rancer, 1994; Shultz & Anderson, 1984). Infante (1989) examined whether males and females responded differently to highly argumentative others, and observed that females become more argumentative with verbally aggressive others, while males tended to rely more on verbal aggressiveness. Several explanations have been offered to describe sex differences in argumentativeness. One of the explanations is the perception of sex role in conflictual situations. For example, Nicotera and Rancer (1994) examined how 24 perceptions of sex role affect argumentative behavior. They found that there were significant differences between males and females on both self-reported and stereotyped perceptions of argumentativeness. Also, Rancer and Dierks- Stewart (1985) examined both biological and psychological gender differences in argumentativeness and observed no differences based on biological sex, but found significant differences in argumentativeness based on psychological gender perception. Namely, Individuals who self-reported as instrumental (masculine) were higher in argumentativeness than those who identified as expressive (feminine). Similarly, Rancer and Baukus (1987) explained that gender differences in argumentativeness are based on individual’s beliefs about arguing. They found that significantly more females than males believed that arguing was a hostile behavior used to control or dominate others. Hence, the following hypothesis is warranted. H4: Males will be more argumentative than females in all three countries; Japan, China, and the U. S. Assertixeness Alberti and Emmons (1978) conceptualized the assertiveness as “people acting in their own best interests, defending their rights without undue anxiety, expressing 25 honest feelings comfortably, and exercising their rights without denying others' rights”(p. 2). Similarly, Thompson, Ishii, and Klopf (1990) defined assertiveness as “an individual’s ability to make requests, actively disagree, express positive or negative personal rights and feelings, initiate, maintain, or disengage from conversations, and stand up for one’s own self without attacking others”(p.829). Assertiveness also should be differentiated from aggression such as verbal aggressive behavior in that it is constructive in nature. Assertiveness refers to maintaining your own rights, stands without inflicting any pain on others, whereas “aggressive people express their feelings, needs, and ideas at the expense of others”(Thompson, Klopf, & Ishii, 1991, p.166). Aasertiveness_and_gulturel Several studies have examined differences in assertiveness between Americans and Japanese. Various studies have indicated that Americans are more assertive and direct than Japanese (Barnland, 1975; Barnland & Araki, 1985; Nomura & Barnland, 1983; Thompson, Klopf, & Ishii, 1990). In collectivistic cultures, group harmony is valued and conformity to the group or opinion of others is much more respected than in individualistic cultures. Therefore, assertiveness is not seen as a positive behavior. 26 Also, in the context of social conflict, Japanese have shown more preference for the use of less assertive, mitigating tactics (Obuchi & Takahashi. 1994), and also mitigating accounts, such as apologies or excuses and less assertive accounts, such as justifications or denial (Itoi, Ohibushi, and Fukuno, 1996). Sueda and Wiseman (1992) found that when dealing with embarrassment, Americans used strategies such as justification, presentation of the facts and aggression more than Japanese. This finding is consistent with the study by TingeToomey (1988) that concluded individuals in low-context cultures tend to use more autonomy-preserving strategies than individuals in high-context cultures. Nomura and Barnland (1983) compared the interpersonal criticism between Americans and Japanese. They found that Japanese tend to remain silent, tell their remarks to a third person, use nonverbal signs of rejection more often than Americans did. Burnland and Araki (1985) found that Japanese prefer to use nonverbal strategies to express a compliment. Overall, the Japanese preference toward an ambiguous way of communication is supported. Several cross—ethnicity studies in the U.S., however, indicated conflicting conclusions that Asian Americans tend to be less assertive than Caucasians on their self-report 27 measure of assertiveness (Sue & Sue, 1977. Zane et a1. 1991), except that Chinese-American males are as assertive as Caucasians on behavioral measures but not on perception (Sue, Ino & Sue. 1983) In China, self-effacing behavior was preferred (Bond, Leung, & Wan, 1982). They explain that in China, making the self-effacing attribution is considered as a virtue, because it usually leads to harmonious interpersonal relationships. Self-effacing is acting humble, “attributing success to good luck and failure to lack of ability” (Bond et al., 1982, p.158). Self-effacing behavior oftentimes is associated with non—assertiveness. While assertiveness might be valued in an individualistic country like the U.S., non-assertiveness is valued in a collectivistic social context. From these observations, it can be explained that people from collectivistic cultures will be less assertive than people from individualistic cultures, therefore, the following hypothesis is warranted. H5: Japanese and Chinese participants will be less assertive than American participants. Assertiyeness_and_fiex¢ There have been several studies, which examined sex differences in assertive behavior in U.S. Martin and Anderson (1996) examined age and sex differences 28 in aggressive and responsive communication traits. They found that there are significant main effects for sex on assertiveness. They found that American males were significantly more assertive than American females. Dubrin (1991) examined sex difference and gender difference on assertive behavior. Suggesting that the gender difference is greater than sex difference, Dubrin (1991) found that male are perceived as more likely to manipulate situations, and to be assertive compared to females. Similar research was conducted both in Japan and China. Thompson, Ishii and Klopf (1990/1991) observed a significant main effect on assertiveness using the Richmond and McCroskey (1985) Assertiveness-Responsiveness Scale. Hamid (1994) found no significant sex difference on assertiveness for students in Hong Kong, while they found that males scored higher than females. However, Anderson, Martin, Zhong, & West (1997) examined assertiveness in China and found that Chinese males were more assertive than Chinese females. These gender differences are explained through the process of socialization and expectation. Several studies also showed that the socialization process causes American boys to be more assertive than girls (Leonard, 1995; Wall & Holden, 1994). In the U. S., the assertive model is often 29 associated with masculinity (Wildman & Clementz, 1986) and perceived as skillful but not as preferable (Delamater & McNamara, 1991; Kelly et al., 1980). Delamater and McNamara (1991) studied female perceptions of assertiveness in conflict situations, and found that a non-assertive confederate was more preferred than an assertive or even empathetic-assertive confederate. Kelly et al. (1980) examined reactions to assertive and non assertive behavior. They found that even though assertive models were viewed as skilled and able, they were given lower ratings of likeability. Further, female assertive behaviors indicated more negative evaluations than male assertive behaviors. The majority of studies support sex differences in assertive behavior, therefore the following hypothesis is warranted. H6: Male participants will be more assertive than females in all three countries. 30 Chapter 4 Research Questions Research Question 1: Are there any significant differences in verbal aggressiveness among participants in three countries, China, Japan and the United Status? Research Question 2: Are there any significant differences in argumentativeness among participants in three countries, China, Japan and the United Status? Research Question 3: Are there any significant differences in assertiveness among participants in three countries, China, Japan and the United Status? Research Question 4: What gender differences exist in the three communication traits: verbal aggressiveness, argumentativeness, and assertiveness across three countries? Research Question 5: What gender, culture, and interaction differences exists across the three communication traits? 31 Chapter 5 Methods Participants from three countries (i.e. China, Japan and the United States) completed the questionnaire that included three scales with some demographic questions. The questionnaires were distributed and self-reported by the participants. Confirmatory factor analysis, and Pearson correlations were used to examine the scales, and then analysis of variance and independent sample t-tests were used to interpret the data. Ramsinants The sample consisted of 666 undergraduate students from three countries; the People’s Republic of China, Japan and the United States. U.S. American participants were 204 undergraduate students attending a large mid-western university (80 males, 120 females, and 4 were unclassified). American students were given extra credit for participation in this study. The Japanese sample consisted of 282 undergraduate students attending a university located in Tokyo, Japan (106 males, 170 females, and 6 who were unclassified). Japanese participants in this study were those who agreed to 32 participate voluntarily without any credit or compensation. The Chinese sample consisted of 180 undergraduate students, attending a university located in Beijing, China (78 males, 101 females, and 1 who were unclassified). The Chinese participants were paid 8 yuan for their completion of this study. Procedures The participants were asked to consent for their participation in the study and then filled out a questionnaire that included the Verbal Aggressiveness Scale (Infante & Wigley, 1986), the Argumentativeness Scale (Infante & Raner, 1982) and the Assertiveness Scale (Richmond & McCroskey, 1992) with some demographic questions. In addition, a research induction which was part of another study was also administered. Translation The questionnaires were translated from English into the respective languages. To ensure that the translated questionnaires had equivalence with the original ones, the method of back translation was followed (Brislin, 1976). The Japanese and Chinese versions of the questionnaire were translated by a bilingual and then back translated into 33 English by a second bilingual. Discrepancies between the original and the English back-translator were reviewed and then the two translators reviewed the Japanese or Chinese characters for clarity. These revised questionnaires were then piloted with a small sample of graduate students from each of the countries. Instrumentation Ihs_yaIhal_Aggrcssixcn§sa_Sgal§t The Verbal Aggressiveness Scale (Infante & Wigley, 1986) that consists of 20 items including 10 positively worded items and 10 negatively worded items was employed to measure participants’ trait to be verbally aggressive. Items were rated on a five point Likert scale with the larger sums of scores indicating greater verbal aggressiveness, after reflecting negatively worded items. Infante and Wigley (1986) reported .81 internal reliability for this scale. Previous studies have supported the reliability and validity of the scale in the U.S. (Infante, et al., 1989; Infante & Golden, 1985) and in Japan (Suzuki & Rancer, 1994). Ihe_Argnmentatiyenss_$gale* The Argumentativeness Scale (Infante & Rancer, 1982) is employed to measure participants’ trait to be argumentative. Eighteen items (out of 20 original items) were rated on a five point Likert 34 scales with the larger score indicating a greater preference for either approaching arguments or avoiding arguments. Infante and Rancer (1982) reported .91 reliability for argumentative approach items and .86 for argumentative avoidance items. The reliability and validity of this scale has been supported by several previous studies in the U.S. (Infante & Gorden, 1989; Rancer & Infante, 1985; Rancer, Kosverg, & Baukus, 1992) and Japanese samples (Suzuki & Rancer, 1994). Several studies have claimed that the Argumentativeness Scale is, in fact, unidimensional (Boster & Levine, 1988; Kazoleas, 1993, Roberto, 1997). Therefore, this study examined both a unidimensional solution and two- factor solution for the Argumentativeness scale. The_Assertiveness_Sgale4 In this study, a 10 item Assertiveness Scale is employed which is part of Richmond and McCroskey's (1998) Assertiveness—Responsiveness Measure. Richmond and McCroskey (1990) reported a reliability of .88 for assertiveness and .93 for responsiveness, and they are uncorrelated (-. 027). This Assertiveness scale was employed to measure participant’s trait to be assertive. All items were rated on a five point Likert scale with the greater score indicating more assertiveness. Several studies both in 35 the U.S. and in other countries have supported the reliability and validity of this scale (Thompson, C. A., Ishii, S., & Klopf, D., 1990; Thompson, C. A., Klopf, D. W. & Ishii, S., 1991). E J' . E J Confirmatory Factor Analysis (Hunter & Gerbing, 1982) was performed on three scales collapsing the country factor. CFA was used to ensure the validity of the scales, as well as to test the unidimensionality of the scale across three countries. This procedure was employed to determine which items of each scale measure the same construct throughout three countries. Therefore, CFA analysis was conducted for three scales separately. Based on the results of the test of internal consistency, items that do not meet the criteria for acceptance are deleted from each scale and this CFA procedure was repeated until acceptable unidimensional solution was reached. CFA analyses resulted in deleting several items from each scale. After performing the CFA procedure, five items remained to comprise the Verbal Aggressiveness Scale. Correlations and errors of items from the Verbal Aggressiveness Scale remaining after tests of internal consistency are presented in table 1. 36 [Table 1 about here] Therefore, five items were used to compute the Verbal Aggressiveness score, with the minimum being 5 and the maximum being 25 points. The Cronbach alpha reliability for this measure was .69 (M212.5, SD=3.8). Originally, the general trait to be argumentative was considered as a multidimensional model having two factors; the tendency to approach arguments and the tendency to avoid arguments. Argumentativeness was conceptualized as the combination of tendency to approach arguments and tendency to avoid arguments, as shown in the equation explained on p.20. However, there are some evidences suggesting that the Argumentativeness Scale be in fact unidimensional (Boster & Lavubnem 1988; Kazoleas, 1993; Roberto, Finucase, 1997). As Roberto and Finucane (1997) argued that, if the argumentativeness scale is multidimensional, “it is inappropriate to sum across the items to compute ARth”, because subtracting ARGav from ARGap means summing up two unrelated construct (p.22). Therefore, the present study conducted CFA analyses both for a one-factor solution and a two-factor solution by reflecting items to see which model apply for the participants of this study. CFA analyses revealed that the Argumentativenesss 37 Scale had a two—factor model; the tendency to approach and the tendency to avoid, which was consistent with the previous findings (Infante & Rancer, 1982; Rancer & Infante, 1985; Rancer, Kosverg, & Baukus, 1992; Suzuki & Rancer, 1994). Therefore, the present study measures the trait argumentativeness using two separate scales, the tendency to approach arguments (ARGap) and the tendency to avoid arguments (ARGav). Again, several items of both factors were deleted based on the tests of internal consistency. Five items were left for the ARGap Scale, or the tendency to approach argument, and four items were left for the ARGav Scale, or the tendency to avoid arguments. Correlations and errors of items from the Argumentativeness Scale remaining after tests of internal consistency are presented in table 2 for ARGap and table 3 for ARGav. [Table 2 and table 3 about here] Five items were summed up to calculate argumentativeness approach score, the minimum being 5 and the maximum being 25, and also four items for the tendency to avoid, the minimum being 5 and the maximum being 20. The Cronbach alpha reliability was .80(M=10.7, SD=3.6) for the 38 ARGap Scale, and .83 (M=15.1, SD=3.9) for the ARGav Scale (Cronbach, 1951). Finally for the Assertiveness Scale, four items were kept after performing CFA procedures. Correlations and errors of items from the Assertiveness Scale left after tests of internal consistency are presented in table 4. [Table 4 about here] Four items were summed up to have an assertiveness score, the minimum being 5 and the maximum being 20. The Cronbach alpha reliability was .80 (M=10.9, SD=4.3) (Cronbach, 1951). Therefore, this study actually deals with four scales, Verbal Aggressiveness, Argumentativeness-Approach (ARGap), Argumentativenee-Avoidance (ARGav), and Assertiveness. 39 Chapter 6 Results Scalflntemrrelatinns Pearson correlations were computed among the three scales to ascertain whether they were measuring what they are supposed to measure. [Table 5 about here] Verbal aggressiveness and both ARGap and ARGav are expected to correlate negatively, because verbal aggressiveness is differentiated from an argumentative approach and also a argumentative avoidance. Verbal aggressiveness is correlated positively with ARGap(r =.128, p<.01), and positively with Assertiveness(r=.088, p<.05) as expected, but also positively with ARGav(r=.153, p<.01) which was not expected. Assertiveness is expected to correlate positively with ARGap and negatively with ARGav and positively with verbal aggressiveness. The results showed that Assertiveness is correlated positively with ARGap(r=.263, p<.01) and correlated negatively with ARGav(r=-.147, p<.01) as expected. As expected, ARGap and ARGap were highly 40 negatively correlated(r=-.459, p<.01), which was the largest intercorrelation among four scales. The only unexpected correlation was the one between Verbal Aggressiveness and ARG-avoidance. However, we might be able to reason that both avoidance and verbal aggressiveness were associated with one’s inability to argue by logic, or reasoning. Because people are not competent in argument in some situations, they tend to appeal to avoidance and maybe verbal aggression. This correlation hints to us that avoidance strategy is associated with verbal aggressiveness in some way. Overall these correlation figures were small enough in magnitude to conclude that the scales were measuring different constructs, though there are some overlaps. First of all, the descriptive statistics (i.e. Means, Standard Deviations, and Sample Size.) for four scales, the Verbal Aggressiveness, the ARGap Scale, the ARGav scale and the Assertiveness scale are examined. The means of all four scales was displayed for the three countries combined, sorted by three countries, and sorted by countries and by sex. [Table 6, 7, 8 and 9 about here] 41 Inn;may_ANQEA_rssults_snuntr¥_hx_ssx To examine country and sex main effect and country by sex interaction effect, a two-way analysis of variance was conducted. Results of the Two-way analysis of variance are presented in table 10, 12, 14, and 16. Line graphs of the mean score for all four scales sort by both country and sex are presented in figure 1-4. Ths_ysrbal_Assressiysnsss_Scalei TWO-way ANOVA results for the Verbal Aggressiveness showed a significant interaction effect (F[2, 628]=11.297, p<.0001, n2=.o23), with main effects for country (F[2,628]=135.103, p<.0001, n2=.28) and sex (F[1,628]=21.21, P<.0001, n2=.022). This interaction effect shows that the sex difference in U.S. is greater than in Japan and China, as clearly shown in figure 1. [Table 10, Figure 1 about here] W In order to find out which country caused this difference, the independent sample t-tests for each. pair of countries were conducted. The verbal aggressiveness scores for Chinese (M=15.66, SD=2.918) are significantly higher than those for Americans (M=12.36, SD 3.569) and Japanese (M=10.62, SD=2.951). Also, scores for Americans are significantly higher than their Japanese 42 counterparts. [Table 11 about here] Ibe_ARGap_§gale* Two-way ANOVA results for the ARGap showed a significant interaction effect (F[2, 628]=12.998 p<.0001, n2=.038), with main effects for country (F[2,628]=11.965, p<.0001, n2=.035) and sex (F[1,628]=10.189, P<.001, n2=.015). As clearly shown in figure 2, there is no interaction effect between Japan and China. What drives this interaction effect is the sex difference in the U.S. and its relation with Japan and China. [Table 12, Figure 2 about here] W In order to find out which country caused this difference, independent sample t- tests for each pair of country were conducted. The argumentativeness approach score for Japanese (M=15.85, SD=3.922) was significantly higher than those for Americans (M=15.02, SD= 3.973) and Chinese (M=l4.04, SD=3.350). Also, scores for Americans are significantly higher than Chinese counterparts. [Table 13 about here] Ihe_AE§a_y_§_c:_a.J.§_L Two-way ANOVA results for the ARGav showed a significant interaction effect (F[2, 628]=9.031, p<.0001, 43 112:.021), with main effects for country (F[2,628]=82.064, p<.0001, n2=.19) and sex (F[1,628]=39.031, p<.0001, n2=.045). Similar to the ARGap and shown clearly in figure 3, there is IN) or little interaction effect between Japan and China. What drives this interaction effect is the sex difference in the ‘U.S. and. its relation. with. Japan. and China. [Table 14, Figure 3 about here] I:tsst_results_of_sach_conntrx_nairi In order to see which country caused this difference, the independent sample t- test for each pair of countries were conducted. The argumentativeness avoidance score for Chinese (M=12.35, SD=2.891) was significantly higher than for Americans (M=11.62, SD: 3.604) and for Japanese (M=8.791, SD=3.090). Also, scores for Americans are significantly higher than their Japanese counterparts. [Table 15 about here] The—AssertilsnesLSsalei Two-way ANOVA results for the Assertiveness showed a significant interaction effect (F[2, 628]=4.768, p<. 01, n2=.01), with main effects for country (F[2,628]=176.077, p<.0001, n2=.36) and sex (F[1,628]=12.836, p<.0001, n2=.013). There are slight 44 interaction effects, whioh might be caused the sex differences between the U.S. and China. [Table 16, Figure 4 about here] I;test_results_gf_§agh_gguntry;pairg The results of a series of independent sample t-test revealed that the assertiveness score for .American. (M=14.36, SD=3.611) are significantly higher' than. those for .Japanese (M=10.27, SD: 3.540) and Chinese (M=8.111, SD=3.290). Also, the scores for Japanese are significantly higher than the Chinese scores. [Table 17 about here] W I;t§sts_between_gguntry* Male and female scores of the four scales were compared by independent sample t-tests collapsing the country factor. The results of t-tests are presented in Table 18. The results indicated that male participants were significantly more aggressive than female participants across the three countries (t=4.789, p<. 0001). As for ARGap, the male participants scored significantly higher than female participants across the three countries (t=2.744, p< .006). In comparison, the male participants scored significantly less on ARGav than female participants across three countries (t=-5.080, p< .0001). Lastly, male 45 participants were significantly more assertive than female participants (t=2.852, p<. 004). [Table 18 about here] I;tests_within_gguntryé T-tests to compare the mean scores of male and female in each country were conducted. T-test of four scales comparing the means scores of the male and female participants in the United States yielded all significant results. The significant sex differences were displayed in the Verbal Aggressiveness Scale(t=5.424, p<.0001), the ARGap scale(t=5.795, p<.0001), the ARGap scale(t=-.6.491, p<.0001), and the Assertiveness Scale(t=4.437, p<.0001). [Table 19 about here] As for Japan, the Verbal Aggressiveness score showed significant difference (t=3.596, p<.0001). Also, significant differences were displayed in ARGap (t=—3.011, p<. 01) and Assertiveness scale (t=2.308, p<.05). Therefore, except for the ARGap scale, three other scales showed significant sex difference. [Table 20 about here] As for the Chinese, there are no significant differences on all four scales. [Table 21 about here] 46 Chapter 7 Discussion The results of this study showed that there exist differences in aggressive communication traits among three countries; the People’s Republic of China, Japan and the United States. Some of these findings are consistent with former findings and others are not. As for Verbal Aggressiveness, Chinese participants scored highest, followed by American and Japanese participants. Although previous studies have shown no difference on verbal aggressive communication traits between Japanese and Americans (Harman, Klopf, and Ishii, 1990), the present study revealed that Americans are more verbally aggressive than Japanese, with Chinese being more verbally aggressive than either Americans or Japanese. Interestingly, Chinese males and females showed no significant difference in verbal aggressiveness, while Japanese males and American males are more verbally aggressive than Japanese females and American females. The results for these two constructs are consistent with the previous studies which concluded that males tend to be more verbally aggressive than females in the U.S. (Burgoon & Miller, 1983; Infante et al., 1984; 47 Infante & Wigley, 1986; Nicotera & Rancer, 1994) and in Japan (Harman, Klopf, and Ishii, 1990), though not in China. Former studies found that Americans are more argumentative than Japanese (Prunty, Klopf, and Ishii, 1990a, 1990b), Japanese participants in this study were found to have a higher tendency to approach argument (ARGap) than either American or Chinese participants. However, the highest mean score for ARGap was obtained from American males (M=16.91, SD=3.464), and lowest score was from American females (M=13.80, SD=3.834). Therefore as a country, the overall Japanese mean (M=15.85) was higher than the overall American mean (M=15.80), because the American females lowered the mean of the US data. Interestingly, both in the Japanese and the Chinese data, no significant sex differences were displayed, while American males scored significantly higher than American females. It can be assumed that sex role is still reinforced among the participants in the U.S. The results for ARGav were consistent with the results of ARGap. Chinese scored highest on ARGav, followed by Americans and Japanese, which showed clear contrast with the result of ARGap. Japanese scored high on approach and low on avoidance and verbal aggressiveness. It can be said that, at 48 least for the particiapnts of this study, Japanese are in fact very argumentative, though not verbally aggressive. In contrast, the Chinese data was low on approach and high on avoidance and verbal aggression. Both Japanese and Chinese are described as valuing harmony and expected to avoid argument; however, they showed characteristic differences, which suggests the necessity of further research to understand these communication behaviors. Again, significant sex differences is indicated in the American and the Japanese data, while no sex difference was found for the Chinese participants of this study. Another interesting finding is that American females scored highest among the entire group on the ARGav scale (M=12.86, SD=3.497), while Japanese males scored lowest (M=8.068, SD=3.246). This indicates that American females tends avoid arguments more than other groups and Japanese males indicates their willingness to engage in arguments more than other groups. American participants were found to be more assertive than either Japanese or Chinese, and Japanese participants were more assertive than Chinese participants. The data was consistent with previous studies showed that Americans are more assertive than Japanese (Barnland, 1975; Barnland & 49 Araki, 1985; Nomura & Barnland, 1983; Itoi, Ohibushi, and Fukuno, 1996; Ohbuchi & Takahashi, 1994; Thompson, Klopf, & Ishii, 1991). Thompson, Klopf, and Ishii (1990) utilized Richmond and McCroskey’s (1985) assertiveness scales, and compared Japanese and Americans assertiveness- responsiveness. In the replication of Thompson, Klopf, and Ishii (1991), their finding was consistent, Americans are highly more assertive, while Japanese were moderately so. An interesting contrast is that Chinese scored lowest on the Assertiveness scale (M=8.111, SD=3.290), even though they scored highest on the Verbal Aggressiveness Scale (M=15.66, SD=2.918). Inconsistent with Anderson, et al. (1997), Chinese females are not significantly different from Chinese males on their assertiveness scores. Again, American males were significantly higher than American females and so are Japanese participants. Hamid (1994) argued that there is a danger in assuming nonassertiveness as maladaptive in cross-cultural counseling. Hamid (1994) argues that “assertiveness is generally linked to healthy personality adjustment in Western psychology”, but in China (Hong Kong), “people are expected to be deferent to authority (p.127)". Because of its social norm, and political situation, Chinese people put 50 more emphasis on conformity to others and maintenance of harmonious relations, which might lead to non-assertive behavior. It is interesting if we focus this sex difference within country. The largest significant sex differences were found in American data. As for Japanese, significant sex differences were found in three scales except the ARGap scale, though not as significant as the American counterpart. Interestingly, no sex differences were evident among all four scales with Chinese data. As clearly shown in the four figures, we can see the larger sex differences in the American data. How can that be explained? Is there something in individualism that reinforces sex differences? Several researchers claimed that the language silence women (Key, 1975; Lakoff, 1975), as the English language itself favors men placing women on the negative end of continuum (Kaplan, 1986, Spender, 1980). Inconsistent with Anderson, et al. (1997), Chinese females are not significantly different from Chinese males on their assertiveness scores. Again, American males were significantly higher than American females and so are Japanese participants. Hamid (1994) argued that there is a danger in assuming 51 nonassertiveness as maladaptive in cross-cultural counseling. Hamid (1994) argues that “assertiveness is generally linked to healthy personality adjustment in Western psychology”, but in China (Hong Kong), “people are expected to be deferent to authority (p.127)”. Because of its social norm, and political situation, Chinese people put more emphasis on conformity to others and maintenance of harmonious relations, which might lead to non-assertive behavior. I' . I' The finding of this study considered several limitations. A first problem is the measurements of this study. The instruments may be culturally biased. When scales are administered to people from different cultural backgrounds, assumptions that underlie items might not hold true. Across cultures, for example, what Americans understands as verbally aggressive may have little overlap with what Japanese and Chinese perceive. Other sampling problem is that all the subjects recruited in this study are students, as with most of previous studies in general. Students as subjects make the population limited in terms of region, age, and socio—economical status. This should be one caution for generalization. Lastly, sex research has 52 been of great interest to many researchers. According to the meta-analysis of Canary and Hause (1993), however, there are virtually no sex differences and sex differences are mostly moderated by a number of factors. Canary and Hause (1993) warned that we rely too much on stereotypes and caused polarization of sexes, and called for more theory on sex and communication. It is suggested psychological and sociological gender should be used rather than biological sex (Canary & Hause, 1993; Dubrin, 1991). A second possible problem is other mediating factors. The biggest problem of all is the social desirability bias (Nicotera, 1990). Nicotera(1993) examined the relationship between argumentativeness and social desirability biases. Nicotera (1993) observed that sex difference diminished after removing social desirability biases and suggested future research of the effect of social desirability. Especially, in intercultural research, social desirability bias complicates data, because socially desirable behavior itself differs between cultures and also some countries focus more on socially desirable activities than others. Hamid (1994) argues that in the U.S. assertiveness is seen as one character of a well—adjusted person, while in China, non-assertiveness is more socially desirable. As he 53 points out, the communicative behavior valued in one culture is not always valued in the same way in another culture. The finding of this study calls for more research on cross- cultural differences in the meaning of aggression and aggressive behavior including verbal aggressiveness, argumentativeness and assertiveness. Another important mediating factor is situation. Trait theorists have been criticized by situationists. Traits of one's behavior are studied with the assumption that our behavior is somewhat consistent across situations. People tend to use the expression like “she is like that” or “he is not usually like this”. This view is based on the trait theories. On the other hand, situationists contend that we behave differently across situations, and situations will determine our behaviors. The argument is over to what extent our behavior can be explained by our traits and or by situations. This view of including both individual’s traits and situational variables is called the interaction approach (Infante, et al. 1990, 1993). It has been recommended that communication research should take an interaction approach (Andersen, 1987; Infante, 1987). It is argued that Infante and Rancer’s (1982) model of argumentativeness takes an interactionist position in that it includes influences of 54 situational variables. This study, however, takes the traits approach, in that it measured aggressive communication traits of people from three countries. Therefore future study can examine the aggressive communication behavior taking situation specific factors into consideration. Actually, not so many studies have been done in Mainland China because of its past political situation. Those previous studies that are mentioned in this study for Chinese were mostly conducted in Hong Kong, Taiwan or Singapore. Thus, more research about Mainland China would be very beneficial in cross-cultural research. Japan and China, both classified as collectivistic cultures, showed significant differences in their communication traits. As we see from the significant differences between Japan and China, countries classified as collectivistic can exhibit differences and need to be examined further. Investigating the effect of culture and the respondents’ gender on aggressive communication traits yielded several important findings. This study provided an attempt to understand three aggressive communication traits (i.e. Verbal Aggressiveness, Argumentativeness Approach and Avoidance, and Assertiveness) of three countries, China, Japan and the U.S. Hopefully, the results might help us to 55 have a better understanding of cultural differences in communication behavior in arguing situation. 56 REFERENCES 57 References Alberti, R. E., & Emmons, M. L. (1978). Xgur_perfegt righti_A_guide_to_asssrtixe_hshayior. San Luis Obispo, CA Impact Publishers. Anderson, C. M., Martin, M. M., Zhong, M., & West, D. (1997). Reliability, separation of factors, and sex differences on the assertiveness-responsiveness measure: A Chinese sample. Cnmmunication_Rsssarsh_RsnsrtsI_14. 58-64. Anderson, P. A. (1987). The trait debate: A critical examination of the individual differences paradigm in interpersonal communication. In E. Dervin & M. J. Voigt (Eds.). Progrsss_in_communication_ssiences (Vol.7, pP-47- 52.) Norwood, NJ: Ablex. Barnland. D. (1975). 2ubIic_and_nrirats_sslf_in_ianan and_ths_united_Statss- Tokyo, Japan: Simul PreSS- Barnland, D., & Araki, S. (1985). Intercultural encounters: The management of compliments by Japanese and Americans. Qournal_of_Cross:Cultural_£s¥sholos¥+_1§. 9-26. Bayer, C. L. & Cegala, D. J. (1992). Trait verbal aggressiveness and arugmentativeness: relations with Parenting style. flsstsrn_Qournal_of_CQmmunisationx_56. 301- 310. Bond, M. H., Leung, K., & Wan, K.C.(1982). The social impact of self-effacing attributions: The Chinese case. The Journal_of_Social_Es¥sholog¥x_llfl. 157-166. Boster, F. J., & Levine, T. (1988). Individual differences and compliance gaining message selection: The effects of verbal aggressiveness, argumentativeness, dogmatism. and negativism. Communication_Rsssarsh_RenQrtsi 5, 114-119. Brislin, R. W. (1986). The wording and translation of research instruments. In W. J. Lonner & J. W. Berry (Eds.). Eisld_msthods_in_cross;sultnral_rsssarch (pp.155-169). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. 58 Burgoon, M., & Miller, G. R. (1983). An expectancy interpretation of language and persuasion. In H. Giles & R. St. Clair (Eds.), Rsssnt_adMancss_in_languassr_communisation and_sogial_psyghglggy.(pp. 199-229) London: Erlbaum. Canary, D. J., & Hause, K. S. (1993). Is there any reason to research sex difference in communication? Communisation_0uartsrlxr_41. 129-144. Cronbach, L. J. (1951). Coefficient alpha and the internal structure of tests. Psychometrikar_1§, 297—334. Delameter, R. J., & McNamara, R. R. (1991). Perceptions of assertiveness by women involved in a conflict situation. BehaMior_Modifisationi_151211 173-193. Dubrin, A. J. (1991). Sex and gender differences in tactics of influence. Esyghglggigal_3epgrtsL_§8, 635—646. Folger, J. P., Poole, M. S., & Stutman, R. K. (1993). Horkins_through_conflisti_12nd_sdil. New York: Harper Collins. Geen, R. G., & Donnerstein, E. L. (Eds.) (1983). Assrsssioni_Ihsorstital_and_smnirisal_re112ws_Ileill. New York: Academic Press. Geertz, C. L. (1973). Ihs_intsrnrstation_sf_culture. New York: Basic Books. Gudykunst, W. E., Ting-Toomey, S., & Nishida, T. (1996). Communications_in_psrsonal_relationshins_across cultures; Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Gudykunst, W. B.,& Matsumoto, Y.(1996). Cross-cultural Variability of Communication in Personal Relationships. In W. B. Gudykunst, S. Ting-Toomey, & T. Nishida (Eds.), I O O I 01111.. o. .0 O 9‘ .090. ‘ o. ,OQ‘Q 0‘ ._ .“ . .‘, (pp. 19-56)Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Hall, E.T. (1976). Beyond_§ulture. New York: Doubleday. Hamid, P. N. (1994). Assertiveness and personality dimensions in Chinese students. Bsygholggigal_RepQrtst_lir 59 127-130. Harman, C. M., Klopf, D. W., & Ishii, S. (1990). Verbal aggression among Japanese and American students. Esrcsntual_and_Motor_Skillsi_19.1130. Hofstede, G. (1980/1984). Culture:s.§onssqnensesi IntsrnatiQnal_diffsrencss_in_nsrk;relatsd_ralnss. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. Hofstede, G. (1983). Dimensions of national cultures in fifty countries and three regions. In J. Deregowski, S. quirawiec, & R. Annis (Eds.), Explisations_in_sross; cultural_nsxchologxi (pp. 335-355) Lisse, The Netherlands: Swets & Seitlinger. Hofstede, G., & Bond,M.(1984). Hofstede’s Culture dimensions: An independent validation using Rokeach’s value survey. Jonrnal_of_Cross:Cultural_ns¥sholos¥i_15. 417-433. Hofstede. G. (1991). Culturss_and_organizationsi Softwars_of_ths_mind. London: McGraw-Hill. Hui, C. H. (1988). Measurement of Individualism— Collectivism: A study of cross-cultural researchers. Journal of_CrQss;Cultural_£s¥cholos¥i_11. 225-248. Hunter, J. E., & Gerbing, D. W. (1982). Unidimensional measurement, second order factor analysis, and causal models.In B. M. Staw & L. L. Cunnings (Eds.), researgh_in Qrsanization_Bshaxior_lei4 (pp.267-320). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. Infante, D. A. (1987). Enhancing the prediction of response to a communication situation from communication traits. Communication_Quarterl¥i_35. 305-316. Infante, D. A. (1989). Response to high argumentatives: Message and sex differences. Southern Communicatinn_lnurnali_54. 159-170. Infante, D. A., & Golden, W. I. (1985). Superiors’ argumentativeness and verbal aggressiveness as predictors of subordinates’ satisfaction. Communication_Quartsrl¥+_31. 81- 9o. 60 Infante, D. A., & Golden, W. I. (1989). Argumentativeness and affirming communicator style as predictors of satisfaction/dissatisfaction with subordinates. Human_Communisation_3sssarshr_12. 117-125. Infante, D. A., & Rancer, A. S. (1982). A conceptualization and measure of argumentativenes. Journal W: 72'80- Infante, D. A., & Rancer, A. S. (1996). Argumentativeness and verbal aggressiveness: A review of recent theory and research. In B. R. Burleson. & A. W. Kunkle. (Eds.), Communication_1earbook_12 (pp.319-352). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Infante, D. A. Rancer, A. S. & Womack, D. F. (1993). Building_sommunisstion_theor¥_12nd_Edil PrOSPeCt Hights, IL: Waveland. Infante, D. A., Trebing, J. D. Shepherd, P. E. & Seeds, D. E. (1984). The relationship of argumentativeness to verbal aggression Southsrn_Snsssh_Communication_flournalr 5o, 67- 77. Infante, D. A., Wall, C. H., Leap, C. J., & Danielson, K. (1984). Verbal Aggression as a function of the receiver’s argumentativeness. Communication_Ressarch_Renortsr_1. 33-37. Infante, D. A., & Wigley, C. J. (1986). Verbal aggressiveness: An interpersonal model and measure. Communication_Monogranhsr_53. 61-69. Itoi, R., Ohibushi, K., & Fukuno, M. (1996). A cross- cultural study of preference of accounts: Relationship closeness, harm severity, and motives of account making. Journal_of_Annlisd_Social_Bs¥cholog¥i_2§, 913-934. Kazoleas, D. (1993). The impact of argumentativeness on resistance to persuasion. Human_Communisation_Rsssarchi 29, 118-137. Kaplan, C. (1986). ssa_changesi_sssars_on_culturs_and fominismr London : Verso. 61 Kelly, J. A., Kern, J. M., Kirkley, B. G., Patterson, J. N., & Keanse, T. M. (1980). Reactions to assertive versus unassertive behavior: Differential effects for males and females and implications for assertiveness training. Beharior_1hsranxi_ll. 670-682. Key. R. M. (1975). MaleLfsmale_languagsi_with_a CQmprohonsiyo_bibliograohyr Metuchen, N.J.:Scarecrow Press. Kim, M. S. (1994). Cross-cultural comparisons of the perceived importance of conversational constraints. Human Communication_RessarshI_21. 128-151. Kim, M. S., & Wilson, S. R. (1994). A cross-cultural comparison of implicit theories of requesting. Communioation MQnQQIaDth_§l, 210-235- Kinney, T. A. (1994). An inductively derived typology of verbal aggression and its association to distress. Human Communication_3essarshr_21. 183-222. Lakoff, T. R. (1975). Languags_and_woman1s_21acs. New York : Harper & Row. Leonard, R. (1995). “I’m just a girl who can’t say ‘no’”: A gender difference in children’s perceptions of refusals. Eeminism_and_2s¥chologxr_5, 315-328. Martin, M. M., & Anderson, C. M. (1996). Communication traits: A cross-generalization investigation. Communroarion Research_Renortsi_13. 58-67. Martin, J. N., & Nakayama, T. K. (1996). Intoroultural communisation_in_contextss Mountain View, California: Mayfield Publishing Company. McCroskey, J. C., & Richmond, V. P. (1982). Eundamsntals_of_human_sommunication. Prospect Heights, IL: Waveland Press.S Nicotera, A. M.(1996). An assessment of the argumentativeness scale for social desirability bias. Communication_8snortsi_2. 23-35. Nicotera, A. M., Rancer, A. S. (1994). The influence 62 of sex of self-perceptions and social stereotyping of aggressive communication predispositions. flootorn_Journal_of communication1_58. 283-307. Nomura, N., & Barnland, D. (1983). Patterns of interpersonal criticism in Japan and the United States. Intsrnational_Journal_of_Intsrcultural_Relations1_1. 1- 17 Ohbuchi, K., & Takahashi, Y. (1994). Cultural styles of conflict management in Japanese and Americans: Passivity, covertness, and effectiveness of strategies. Journal_of Apnlisd.$oc1a1_2s¥sholog¥1_24, 1345-1366. Prunty, A. M., Klopf, D. W., & Ishii, S. (1990a). Argumentativeness: Japanese and American tendencies to approach and avoid conflict. Communication_Rsssarch_Rsnorts1 1, 75-79. Prunty, A. M. Klopf, D. M. & Ishii, S. (1990b). Japanese and American tendencies to argue. Esyohologioal Reports1_6§. 802. Rancer, A. S., & Burkus, R. A. (1987). Discriminating males and females on belief structures about arguing. In L. B. Nadler, M. K. Nadler, & W, R. Todd- Mancillas (Eds. ), AdMancss_in_gsndsr_and_communication_rsssarsh (pp. 155- 173). Lanham, MD: University Press of America. Rancer, A. M., & Dierks-Stewart, K. J. (1985). The influence of sex and sex-role orientation on trait argumentativeness, Qournal_of_Psrsonalit¥_Assessmsnts1_49, 69-70. Rancer, A. M., & Infante, D. A. (1985). Relations between motivation to argue and the argumentativeness of adversaries. Communisation10uarterl¥1_33, 209-218. Rancer, A. S., Kosberg, R. L., & Baukus, R. A. (1992). Beliefs about arguing as predictors of trait argumentativeness: Implications for training in argument and conflict management. Communication_Education1_41. 375-387. Richmond, V. P., & McCroskey, J. C. (1985/1992) I O O C U C . ‘ - - . ‘ . a- A - --- .IIII U ' .. e . C 9- .. v . v. 2. Scottsdale, AZ: Gorsuch Scarisbrick. 63 Richmond, V. P., & McCroskey, J. C. (1990). Reliability and separation of factors on the assertiveness-responsive measure. Psxchological_3snorts1_§1. 449-450 Richmond, V. P., & McCroskey, J. C. (1998). I 0 I O .IHI.l 0. 0!‘ #0.. ‘0‘0‘ .0 o. 0 0.2! ‘ a... ‘ ‘ ‘!‘_. lEifith_oditionL. Boston: Allyn and Bacon. Roberto, A. J., & Finucane, M. (1997). The assessment of argumentativeness and verbal aggressiveness in adolescent pepulations. Communisation_0uartsrl¥1_45. 21-36. Roloff, M. E., & Greenberg, B. S. (1979). Sex differences in choice of modes of conflict resolution in real-life and television. Communroarion_Quarrorlyr_21, 3-12. Samover, L. A., & Porter, R. E. (1994). An introduction to intercultural communication. In L. A. Samovar. & R. E. Porter (Eds.), Intsrcultural_communisation1 a_roador (p.4-26). Belmont, California: Wadsworth Publishing Company. Sanders, J. A., Gass, R. H., Wiseman, R. L., & Bruschke, J. C. (1992). Ethnic Comparison and measurement of argumentativeness, verbal aggressiveness, and need for cognition. Communication_Reports1_5, 50-56. Schultz, E., & Anderson, J. (1984). Training in the management of conflict: A communication theory perspective. .Small_Group_Bshayior1_15. 333-348. Spender, D. (1980). Man_mado_languagor Boston Routledge & Kegan Paul. Sue, D., Ino, S., & Sue, D.M. (1983). Nonassertiveness of Asian Americans: An inaccurate assumption? Journal of Counseling Psychology, 30, 581-588. Sue, D. W., & Sue, D. (1977). Barriers to effective cross-cultural counseling. Journal_of_Counssling_Esxchologr1 23, 420-429. Sueda, K., & Wiseman, R. (1992). Embarrassment remediation in Japan and the United States. lntornational 64 Journal_of_Intsrcultural_Rslations1_18121. 193-219. Thompson, C. A., Ishii, S., & Klopf, D. (1990). Japanese and Americans compared on assertiveness /re8ponsiveness. Psxchologisal_rsnorts1_66. 829-830. Thompson, C. A., Klopf, D. W., & Ishii, S. (1991). A comparison of social style between Japanese and Americans. Communication_Research_Reports1_8, 165-172. Ting—Toomey, S. (1988). Intercultural conflict styles: A face-negotiation theory. In Y.Kim & W. Gudykunst (Eds.), Iheoriss_1n_1ntsrsultural_communication1(pp. 213-235). Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Ting-Toomey, S., Gao, G., Trubisky, P., Yang, Z., Kim, H. S., Lin, 8., and Nishida, T. (1991). Culture, face maintenance, and styles of handling interpersonal conflict: a study in five cultures. Iha_lntornational_flournal_ofi Conflict_Management1_2. 275-296. Triandis, H. C. (1988). Collectivism vs. Individualism: A reconceptualization of a basic concept in cross-cultural psychology. In G. Verma & C. Bagley (Eds.), Cross;cultural_stud1ss_of_nersonalit¥1_att1tudss1_and oognition_(pp.70-95). London: Macmillan. Triandis, H. c. (1995). Indiridualism_&_Collectiyism. San Francisco, CA: Westview Press Triandis, H. C., Bontempo, R., Villareal, M. J., Asai, M., & Lucca, N. (1988). Individualism and collectivism: Cross-cultural perspectives on self-ingroup relations. 1ournal_of_Psrsonal1tM_and_Social_£s¥sholog¥1_54. 323-338. Triandis, H. C., McCusker, C., & Hui, C. H. (1990). Multimethods probes of individualism and collectivism. Qournal_of_Rsrsonal1tx_and1Social_£s¥sholog¥1_52. 1006-1020. Wall, J. E., & Holden, E. W. (1994). Aggressive, assertive, and submissive behaviors in disadvantaged, inner- city preschool children. Qournal_of_§linioal_ghild Esrcholog¥1_23, 382-390. Wildman, B. G., & Clements, B. (1986). Assertive, 65 empathic assertive, and conversational behavior: perception of likability, effectiveness and sex role. Bohauior Modification1_19. 315-331. Zane, N. S., Sue, S., Hu, L., & Kwon, J. H.(l991). Asian-American assertion: A social learning analysis of cultural differences. Qournal_of_counssling_ns¥sholog¥1_38. 63-70. 66 APPENDICES 67 Appendix A Tables 68 Tables 1: Correlations and errors of the Verbal Aggressiveness Scale items left after the test of Internal Consistency* 7 11 13 16 19 7 .01 -.04 -.04 .00 11 .33 -.03 .02 -.01 13 .22 .38 .01 -.01 16 .27 .28 .24 .01 19 .32 .44 .34 .27 F 50 68 .52 45 64 *Bold numbers are the indicators in the original scale. *The numbers in the lower triangle are obtained correlations. The numbers in the upper triangle with italicized figures represent errors. Errors are calculated by the subtraction of obtained correlation from predicted correlation. 69 Tables 2: Correlations and errors of the ARGap Scale items left after the test of Internal Consistency* 6 12 14 15 18 6 .01 -.03 -.01 .00 12 .34 -.03 .02 -.03 14 .44 .55 .00 .02 15 .44 .53 .64 -.03 18 .38 .51 .54 .63 F 53 67 .78 83 73 *Bold numbers are the indicators in the original scale. *The numbers in the lower triangle are obtained correlations. The numbers in the upper triangle with italicized figures represent errors. Errors are calculated by the subtraction of obtained correlation from predicted correlation. 70 Tables 3: Correlations and errors of the ARGav Scale items left after the test of lntemal Consistency* 3 11 17 13 3 .00 -.03 .02 11 .47 .02 -.02 17 .54 .50 .01 13 .46 .52 .52 *Bold numbers are the indicators in the original scale. *The numbers in the lower triangle are obtained correlations. The numbers in the upper triangle with italicized figures represents errors. Errors are calculated by the subtraction of obtained correlation from predicted correlation. 71 Tables 4: Correlations and errors of the Assertiveness Scale items left after the test of Internal Consistency* 6 8 9 10 6 - 02 .00 01 8 .58 .01 -.04 9 60 .62 - 02 10 .46 .50 .55 *Bold numbers are the indicators in the original scale. *The numbers in the lower triangle are obtained correlations. The numbers in the upper triangle with italicized figures represent errors. Errors are calculated by the subtraction of obtained correlation from predicted correlation. 72 Table 5: Scale Intercorrelations of four scales AGGRESS ARGap ARGap ASSERT AGGRESS 1 .000 ARGap ".128 1 .000 ARGav **.1 53 **-.459 1 .000 ASSERT *.088 ”.263 **-.147 1 .000 ** Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed). * Correlation is significant at the 0.05 level (2-tailed). 73 Table 6: Descriptive Statistics (Mean, Standard Deviation, and Sample Size) of the Verbal Aggressiveness Scale Mean Std .' Dev. N U.S. overall 12.36 3.56 220 male 13.94 3.39 79 female 1 1.33 3.83 1 19 Japan overall 10.65 2.95 278 male 11.39 3.41 140 female 10.09 2.52 168 China overall 15.66 2.91 180 male 15.42 3.12 101 female 15.79 2.71 78 Three overall 12.53 3.75 660 countries Combined male 13.37 3.72 261 female 1 1.95 3.65 388 This scale’s maximum total score is 25, minimum being 5. 74 Table 7: Descriptive Statistics (Mean, Standard Deviation, and Sample Size) of ARGap Scale Mean Std. Dev. N U.S. overall 15.02 3.97 200 male 16.91 3.46 79 female 13.80 3.83 1 19 Japan overall 15.85 3.92 270 male 15.80 3.97 104 female 15.88 3.91 165 China overall 14.04 3.35 180 male 13.98 3.59 78 female 14.01 3.10 101 Three overall 15.09 3.85 650 countries Combined male 15.59 3.87 261 female 14.75 3.81 385 This scale’s maximum total score is 25 minimum being 5. 75 Table 8: Descriptive Statistics (Mean, Standard Deviation, and Sample Size) of the ARGav Scale Mean Std. Dev. N U.S. overall 11.62 3.69 202 male 9.75 2.98 79 female 12.86 3.49 120 Japan overall 8.791 3.09 268 male 8.068 3.24 102 female 9.22 2.91 165 China overall 12.35 2.89 180 male 12.10 3.02 78 female 12.58 2.76 101 Three overall 10.65 3.58 645 countries Combined male 9.79 3.51 259 female 1 1.23 3.52 386 This scale’s maximum total score is 20 minimum being 4. 76 Table 9: Descriptive Statistics (Mean, Standard Deviation, and Sample Size) of the Assertiveness Scale Mean Std. Dev. N U.S. overall 14.36 3.611 202 male 15.71 2.98 80 female 13.50 3.675 1 19 Japan overall 10.27 3.54 272 male 10.89 3.444 104 female 9.880 3.56 167 China overall 8.1 1 1 3.29 180 male 7.974 3.47 78 female 8.099 2.94 101 Three overall 10.91 4.25 649 countries Combined male 1 1.49 4.51 262 female 10.52 4.03 387 This scale’s maximum total score is 20 minimum being 4. 77 Table 10: Two-way ANOVA results of the Verbal Aggressiveness Scale country by sex AGRESS :quu'grgg Of 533:; F Sig. 112 Country 2428.341 2 1241.171 135.103 .000 .273 Sex 194.873 1 194.873 21.212 .000 .022 “£31 by 207.588 2 103.783 11.297 .000 .023 Residual 5723.419 823 9.187 Total 8893.872 828 14.182 Table 1 1: T-test result of the Verbal Aggressiveness scales comparing three pairs of countries T score Df Sig. “use? -..... vséilianrin -1 1889 456 .000 78 Table 12: Two-way ANOVA results of the ARGap Scale country by sex Sum of Mean . 2 ARGAP Squares Df Square F S'g' '1 Country 327.866 2 163.933 11.965 .000 .035 Sex 139.179 1 139.179 10.198 .001 .015 “”823 by 358.179 2 178.089 12.998 .000 .038 Residual 8536.006 623 13.701 Total 9348.754 628 14.887 Table 13: T-test result of the ARGap scales comparing three three pairs of countries T score 01 Sig. ”33.535323” 12335 “2 ‘°°° .. vségilnpaan 65“ 450 .000 79 Table 14: Two-way ANOVA results of the ARGap scale country by sex ARGav 88:02:; Df Shcdifizre F Sig. Ti 2 Country 1588.145 2 784.072 82.084 .000 .193 Sex 327.912 1 372.912 39.030 .000 .040 muggy by 172.583 2 88.281 45.384 .000 .021 Residual 5952.382 823 9.554 Total 8119.485 828 12.929 Table 15: T-test result of the ARGav scales comparing three three pairs of countries T score Df Sig. United States 9.170 468 .000 vs.Japan United States -2.140 380 .033 vs. China China -12.260 446 .000 vs.Japan 80 Table 16: Two-way ANOVA results of the Assertiveness Scale country by sex ASSERT Ssquu'grgg Df sac/:32; F Sig. 11’ Country 4139.078 2 2089.538 178.077 .000 .358 Sex 150.885 1 150.885 12.838 .000 .013 muggy by 112.080 2 58.040 4.788 .009 .010 Residual 7322.481 623 11.754 Total 11817.701 628 18.500 Table 17: T-test result of the Assertiveness Scale comparing three three pairs of countries T score 01‘ Sig. Unlted States -2.261 468 .024 vs. Japan United States vs. China 2'5” 372 '010 China vs. Japan 5070 448 .000 81 Table 18: T-test results of four scales between males and females across three countries T score Df Sig. Aggress 4.789 647 .000 ARGap 2.744 644 .006 ARGav -5.080 643 .000 Assert 2.852 647 .004 82 Table 19: T-test results of four scales between males and females in the United States U. S. T score Df Sig. Aggress 5.424 198 .000 ARGap 5.795 196 .000 ARGav -6.491 197 .000 Assert 4.437 197 .000 Table 20: T-test results of four scales between males and females In Japan Japan T score Df Sig. Aggress 3.596 270 .000 ARGap -.1 57 267 .876 ARGav -3.01 1 265 .003 Assert 2.308 269 .022 Table 21: T-test results of four scales between males and females in China China T score Df Sig. Aggress -.844 1 17 .400 ARGap -.065 1 17 .948 ARGav -1.110 117 .269 Assert -.260 1 17 .795 83 Appendix B Figures 84 Figure1: Line graph of mean scores for the Verbal Aggressiveness Scale by country and sex l l /' 13« 124 (I) (1) LL! 8:9 111 SEX $2 " E 104 ' male 0 E 9 . ‘ female US JAPAN CHINA COUNTRY Figure2: Line graph of mean scores for the ARGap Scale by country and sex 17.5 17.01 16.5- 16.0 - 15.54 15.0« 14.5 1 14.0 1 Mean ARG AP 13.5 i - us JAPAN CHINA COUNTRY 85 Figure 3: Line graph of mean scores for the ARGav Scale by country and sex a .5 O) -F _ - - ...._.._7 1.... _ .___. __ 1 A N .-1 an. A l—_.__. .11.... .. I I 15 I l \ \ 1..-- “1-11.--- -1-.- _._1____\__.\ _-___-_ .___t ‘0 Mean ARG AV m | l l l i .1 l 7 US JAPAN CHINA COUNTRY Figure 4: Line graph of mean scores for the Assertiveness Scale by country and sex 18 16-1 Mean ASSERT COUNTRY 86 Appendix C Questionnaires and Research Consent Form 87 Directions: The following questions are concerned with how we try to get people to comply with our wishes. Indicate how often each statement is true for you personally when you try to influence other people. Use the following scale. 1 = almost never true 2 = rarely true 3 = occasionally true 4 = often true 5 = almost always true 10. I am extremely careful to avoid attacking a person's intelligence when l attack their ideas. 11.When individuals are very stubborn I use insults to soften the stubbornness. 12. I try very hard to avoid having people feel bad about themselves when I try to influence them. 13. When people refuse to do a task I know is important, without good reason I tell them they are unreasonable. 14. When other do things I regard as stupid, I try to be extremely gentle with them. 15. If individuals I am trying to influence really deserve it I attack their character. 16. When people behave in ways that are in poor taste, I insult them to shock them into proper behavior. 17. I try to make people feel good about themselves even when their ideas are stupid. 18. When people simply will not budge on a matter of importance I lose my temper and say rather strong things to them. 19. When people criticize my shortcomings, I take it in good humor and do not try to get back at them. 20. When individuals insult me, I get a lot of pleasure out of really telling them off. 21 .When I dislike individuals greatly, I try not to show it in what I day or how I say it. 22. I like poking fun at people who do things which are very in order to stimulate their intelligence. 23. When I attack people's ideas I try not to damage their self-concepts. 24. When I try to influence others, I make a great effort not to offend them. 88 25. When people do things which are mean or cruel, l attack their character in order to help correct their behavior. 26. I refuse to participate in arguments when they involve personal attacks. 27. When nothing seems to work in trying to influence others, I yell and scream in order to try and get some movement from them. 28. When I am not able to refute others' positions, I try to make them feel defensive in order to weaken their positions. 29. When an argument shifts to personal attacks I try hard to Change the subject. Directions: The following scale asks you to evaluate how you feel about a number of issues. After reading each statement, indicate how much you agree or disagree using the following scale. 1 = Strongly Disagree 2 = Disagree 3 = Undecided 4 = Agree 5 = Strongly Agree 30. I should be judged on my own merit. 31. l voice my opinions in group discussions. 32. I feel uncomfortable disagreeing with my group. 33. I conceal my negative emotions so I won’t cause unhappiness in my group. 34. My personal identity, independent of others is very important to me. 35. I prefer to be self-reliant rather than dependent upon others. 36. I act as a unique person, separate from others. 37. I don’t like depending upon others. 38. My relationships with others in my group are more important than my personal accomplishments. 39.My happiness depends on the happiness of those in my group 89 40. I often consider how I can be helpful to specific others in my group. 41. I take responsibility for my own actions. 42. It is very important for me to act as an independent person. 43. l have an opinion about most things; I know what I like, and I know what I don’t like. 44. I enjoy being unique and different from others. 45. I don't change my opinions in conformity with those of the majority. 46. Speaking up in a work/task group is not a problem for me. 47. Having a lively imagination is important to me. 48. Understanding myself is a major goal in my life. 49. I enjoy being admired for my unique qualities. 50. I am careful to maintain harmony in my group. 5 1. When I’m with my group, I watch my words so I won’t offend anyone. 52. I would sacrifice my self-interest for the benefit of my group. 53. I try to meet demands of my group, even if it means controlling my own desires. 54. It is important to consult close friends and get their ideas before making decisions. 55. I should take into consideration my parents’ advice when making education and career plans. 56. I act as fellow group members would prefer me to. 57. The security of being an accepted member of a group is very important to me. 58. If my brother or sister fails, I feel responsible. 9O Directions: The following questions ask you for your opinion about arguing about controversial issues. Indicate how often each statement is true for you personally using the following scale. 1 = statement is almost never true for you 2 = statement is rarely true for you 3 = statement is occasionally true for you 4 = statement is often true for you 5 = statement is always true for you 59. While in an argument, I worry that the person I am arguing with will form a negative impression of me. 60. Arguing over controversial issues improves my intelligence. 61. I enjoy avoiding arguments. 62. Once I finish an argument I promise myself that I will not get into another one. 63. Arguing with a person creates more problems for me than it solves. 64. I have a pleasant, good feeling when I win a point in an argument. 65. When I finish arguing with someone I feel nervous and upset. 66. I enjoy a good argument over a controversial issue. 67. I get an unpleasant feeling when I realize I am about to get into an argument. 68. I enjoy defending my point of view on an issue. 69. I am happy when I keep an argument from happening. 70. I do not like to miss the opportunity to argue a controversial issue. 71. I prefer being with people who rarely disagree with me. 72. I consider an argument an exciting intellectual challenge. 73. I find myself refreshed and satisfied after an argument on a controversial issue. 74. l have the ability to do well in an argument. 75. I try to avoid getting into arguments. 91 76. I feel excitement when I expect that a conversation I am in is leading to an argument. ' Directions: Please indicate the degree to which you believe each of these characteristics applies to you by marking 1 = it doesn't apply to you at all 2 = it only minimally applies to you 3 = you are undecided whether it applies to you 4 = you agree that it applies to you 5 = strongly agree that it applies to you 77. defends own beliefs 78.independent 79. forceful 80. has a strong personality 81. you say what you think 82. dominant 83. willing to take a stand 84. acts as a leader 85. aggressive 86. competitive 87. My gender is 1. male 2. female 88. My age is 1. 17 - 18 2. 19 - 20 3. 21-22 4. 23 or older 92 Research Consent Form The purpose of this study is to assess how you would react to a complaint about your behavior made by a neighbor. It will take approximately 40 minutes to complete this study. When you are done we ask that you remain quietly in your seat until everyone has completed the study. 1. Apart from your participation in this study, your actual performance in this study will in no way affect your evaluation in this course or in any other course. 2. Any credit you may earn via participating in this study is not transferable to any other course or any other semester. 3. Your participation in this study does nor guarantee any beneficial results to you. 4. You have the right to withdraw from this study at any time without penalty. Another assignment will be given to you if you wish to earn the extra credit points. 5. You have the right to have this study explained to you to your satisfaction. 6. The results of this study will be treated with strict confidence with regard to the data of any participant. With this restriction, the results will be made available to you at your request. 7. The data you provide to the researcher may be used by other scientists for secondary analysis. Again data will be treated with strict confidence. 8. Should you have any questions, problems, complaints, or if you desire further information, you have the right to contact the following person. Mary Bresnahan Department of Communication Michigan State University ******************************************************** Given these understandings, l voluntarily agree to participate in this research as descnbed. Print your name |D# 93 "I11111111111111