llllllllilIlllllllllllllllllllllllllllllHlllllllllllllllllllil 193 02079 6417 This is to certify that the dissertation entitled THE USE OF SELECTED COPING STRATEGIES AS PREDICTORS OF ACADEMIC PERFORMANCE IN MEDICAL SCHOOL presented by Norma Irene Baptista has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for Joctoral—degree in ——Education— Major professor Date 4-21- 1999 MS U is an Affirmative Action/Equal Opportunity Institution 0- 12771 _ h.— .7 - ‘+* _‘ ”.v ‘ ,_._ LIBRARY Michigan State University PLACE IN RETURN BOX to remove this checkout from your record. TO AVOID FINES return on or before date due. MAY BE RECALLED with earlier due date if requested. DATE DUE DATE DUE DATE DUE I «3% 9153200 “ 11/00 mus-m4 THE USE OF SELECTED COPING STRATEGIES AS PREDICTORS OF ACADEMIC PERFORMANCE IN MEDICAL SCHOOL By Norma Irene Baptism DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Education 1999 ABSTRACT THE USE OF SELECTED COPING STRATEGIES AS PREDICTORS OF ACADEMIC PERFORMANCE IN MEDICAL SCHOOL By Norma Irene Baptista Stress among medical students has been linked to poor academic performance, while supportive social relationships have been associated with the alleviation of psychological stress. This study examines students' coping mechanisms and social support as predicrors of academic performance. A tOtal of 73 students from the College of Human Medicine at Michigan State University participated in the study. The Coping Response Inventory (CRI-Adult) was the data collection procedure used for this study. This inventory provides areas for the students to record demographic information, description of a problem or situation they experienced within the put 12 months, and their responses to 48 items related to that specific situation. The problems the students described were categorized as follows: academic performance, workload, adjustment to medical school, family relationships, social relationships, health, financial constraints, multiple events and other or unspecified. The Independent Sample T-TeSt and the Levene's teSt at a significant level of P>.05 were used to test the Study's hypothesis. The data show that the major stressors for the students were workload, academic performance and social relationships. It was found that 25.7% of males reported workload concerns, while 18.4% of females reported having academic difficulties and 18.4% of females also reported having multiple stressors. It was also found that 26.3% of students age 21 to 25 reported social concerns, while 36% of students age 26 to 30 Norma Irene Baptista reported academic difficulty as their main srressor. Additionally, 21% of single students reported social stressors, while 36% of married Students reported workload as their main concern. 26.9% of second year students reported workload as their main source of stress, while 19.1% of first year students reported social Stressors. The statistical teSts results showed: (1) that students reporting academic difficulties used more cognitive avoidance c0ping mechanisms than the students who did n0t report academic difficulties, (2) that females in the sample used more cognitive avoidance coping mechanisms than males, and (3) that Students ages 21 to 25 used more cognitive avoidance coping mechanisms than the older students. GAL! Git RESE TABLE OF CONTENTS CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION Stress and Coping Mechanisms ............................................................... Social Support ........................................................................................... Statement of Purpose ............................................................................... Significance of the Research ..................................................................... Limitations ............................................................................................... Research Questions .................................................................................. Hypothesis ............................................................................................... Definition of Terminology ....................................................................... Organization of the Study ........................................................................ CHAPTER II REVIEW OF RELATED LITERATURE .......................................................... The General Adaptation Theory ............................................................. The Cognitive Appraisal ......................................................................... The Transactional Model ........................................................................ The Main Sources of Stress Experienced by Medical Students and Their Effect on Academic Performance .............................................. CHAPTER III RESEARCH METHODOLOGY ....................................................................... Research Questions ................................................................................. Hypothesis .............................................................................................. Population and Sample ......................................................................... Informed Consent ................................................................................. Data Collection ....................................................................................... Instrumentation ...................................................................................... CRI-Adult Scales .................................................................................... CRI-Adult Scales and Descriptions ........................................................ Reliability ............................................................................................... Organization of the Inventory ............................................................... Criteria for Interpreting CRI-Adult Standard Scores .............................. Academic Performance ....................................................................... Data Analysis ......................................................................................... iv Page 2 5 6 9 9 9 10 11 12 14 14 15 16 24 24 25 26 27 27 27 29 29 30 31 32 32 33 CHAPTERIV DISCUSSION OF FINDINGS ............................................................................. 34 Results of the CRI-Adult Inventory ........................................................ 35 Table 1 .......................................................................................... 35 Table 2 .......................................................................................... 36 Table 3 ......................................................................................... 37 Table 4 ......................................................................................... 38 Table 5 ......................................................................................... 39 Table 6 ......................................................................................... 40 Testing of Hypothesis .............................................................................. 41 CHAPTER V SUMMARY AND RECOMMENDATIONS .................................................... 46 Summary of Findings ................................................................................ 48 Recommendations ..................................................................................... 49 APPENDICES A. Sample Letter ....................................................................................... 53 B. Consent Form ...................................................................................... 55 C. CRI-Adult Answer Sheet .................................................................... 57 Part 1 ............................................................................................ 58 Part 2 ............................................................................................ 59,60 D. CRI-Adult Profile ............................................................................... 62 E. The Pearson Correlation Table 7 ........................................................ 65 Table 8 ......................................................................................... 66 Table 9 ....................................................................................... 67 Table 10 ....................................................................................... 68 Table 11 ....................................................................................... 69 Table 12 ....................................................................................... 70 Table 13 ....................................................................................... 71 Table 14 ...................................................................................... 72 Table 15 ...................................................................................... 73 Table 16 ...................................................................................... 74 BIBLIOGRAPHY ................................................................................................. 7S CHAPTER 1 INTRODUCTION Traditionally, medical education has long been the route to an attractive career. Usually, pre-medical and entering medical students have idealistic views about becoming physicians. They often express their desire to serve humanity or participate in break through research or treatment to eliminate diseases. They also tend to glamorize the physician’s life more than the students in the advanced levels. Some students admit they are attracted to the social position, respect, income and prestige that doctors enjoy. For these reasons and others, medical schools have traditionally received a great number of applicants. From society perspective the selection of students is critical because of the length and the cost of training. Therefore, a considerable amount of attention has been focused on finding accurate and valid predictors of success in medical school. In 1976, The Association of American Colleges compiled the results of a series of studies. The authors, Cuca, Sakakeeny and Johnson, (1976), related medical students’ personality factors, Medical College Achievement Test (MCAT) scores, and premedical Grade Point Averages (GPA’s) to their academic success. The studies suggested that there was not a clear and definitive correlation between these factors and academic success, and that other factors may have intervened. Although none of the studies reviewed identified specific predictors for the academic success of medical students, medical colleges across the country still make their admission decision based heavily on high MCAT scores and high GPA’s. Admissions officers will certainly find solace in Mitchell’s review of recent research validating the use of pre-admission academic data in selecting medical school applicants, (Mitchell, 1990). He presents evidence that the MCAT can be combined with GPA’s for college selectivity, to yield median validity coefficients of 0.49 for pre-clinical (first and second year of medical school) studies and 0.38 for clinical (third and fourth year of medical school) performance at several medical schools. However, Rogers (1989) cautions against over emphasis on psychometric and statistical influences in determining who is to be selected for medical training. He also suggests that medical schools could either select students to match the demands of their curriculum or they can design a curriculum that assures success of those students they seek to attract. Perhaps, a useful approach to the selection of applicants for medical school is the one that includes the assessment of the individual’s ability to deal successfully with highly stressful situations. Stress and Coping Mghagg' ms Stress continues to play an important role in current theoretical approaches to psychological health and disorder. Since the 1960’s, the prevalent research approach to stress and its relation to physical and mental health has been directed at the study of in Chi significant life events or life changes. The measurement of these life events was pioneered by the development of the Schedule of Recent Experiences (SRE) by Holmes and Rahe (1967). Many research studies have utilized this sale for assessing stress in relationship to a wide variety of physical and psychological outcomes (Dohrenwend and Dohrenwend, 1976). Since the original development of the SRE and the studies that followed, many questions have been raised with regard to the psychometric properties of the scale and the adequacy of the research employing it (e.g., Brown and Harris, 1978). The early phases of stress research had a characteristic emphasis on the prediction of illness rates from knowledge of stressful life events and conditions. More recent research has focused on 1) the impact of life events and conditions; 2) the psychological and social situations that determine both the meaning of the events and 3) the individual or group capacities for dealing with stressors. Recent research has also moved away from defining stress in terms of amount of change, per the Schedule of Recent Experiences, to a consideration of other properties of life events or other aspects of context that may be more useful in explaining the meaning and impact of events. Much attention, for example, has been given to questioning the commonsense expectation that events with undesirable implications are more stressful than those with desirable implications. More recently, stressful life events and their role in facilitating life transitions have been closely studied through a structured approach that is intended or directed to understand the ways people handle transitions and manage change (Selye, 1978). "fl Sch: 1:. ft mak. Stress . MC (1985;, Coping (1983) . with 10. Brammer (1990) defines stress as a condition that causes a reaction. In other words, the greater the intensity of the conditions, the greater the chances for these conditions to cause the stress reactions-called stressors. The results of these external stressors leads to stress responses. The intensity of the stress response is related to the strength of the stressors opposed by the strength of the person’s coping resources/ skills. Brammer believes that one key in managing our stressors is the condition of our coping skills. Although stress management includes a cluster of special coping skills, the extent of stress reduction is also a function of our support systems, attitudes about change, problem- solving capacities, thought control, and behavior change methods. According to Schneider (1984) each potential source of stress becomes a stressor only when the person: 1) fails to recognize the event or circumstance stressful; 2) does not fully understand what makes the circumstance stressful; 3) does not see any alternative way to react to the source of stress in order to reduce its impact; 4) currently lacks positive factors in his or her life; and 5) lacks the presence of supportive, ongoing relationships. Schneider also considers that individuals who are able to recognize, understand, and maintain flexibility in the context of ongoing support are able to limit, and often thrive on, the impact of stress on their lives. Most recently, growing research among psychologists, such as Suls and Fletcher (1985), Clark and Hovanitz (1989); and Endler and Parker (1990) has indicated that caping skills help to alleviate health-destroying stressors. Kobassa and Pucetti’s work (1983) with executives indicates that perceived support from superiors was associated with low illness rates. Key protective attitudes uncovered by these studies were hope, Col]e a; positive self-regard, and self-empowerment. These attitudes are postulated to reduce subjects’ sense of helplessness; to serve as buffers against depression, and to facilitate immune responses. Seed—Sum “Social Support” refers to all that is involved in the caring relationships among people. Our embeddeness in a continuing network of such relationships is perhaps what counts most. Gottlieb, Hobfoll and Stokes (1980) focused on social networks, their structure, density and the personal characteristics of the provider and the needy person, and they also identified some approaches to social support to call attention to the transactions occurring in personal relationships. This secure place in one or more networks has profound effect upon how we think and how we feel about our surroundings, and particularly about how we affirm the value of ourselves, (Pilicuk and Hiller Parks, 1986). In general, as social support has been more widely used to describe the help or guidance received by people such as relatives, friends and competent individuals who also provide the culturally expected close personal relationships. The research on social support is profuse and confirms the common sense observation that support is essential to good mental health and psychological growth (Gottlieb, B., Social Networks and Social Support, 1981). Brammer (1990) describes support as the help we receive from friends, relatives, colleagues, and mentors. This support is organized into informal systems called IlfI‘ inter Loci 1 316C i“. “3835f that“ medic; during 4 ”Pen: networks. He also defines support system as a network of all the helpers working for the benefit of the person. Other research, such as Sarason and Sarason (1983), has found significant relationships between social support and life transitions, job performance and improved performances on academic tasks and exams. For this study the researcher will concentrate on the Approach and the Avoidance coping mechanisms. Approach coping mechanisms are problem - focused behavioral coping responses, they include overt actions intended to deal directly with the situation. Individuals using this mechanisms are directed towards dealing with the problem, they intend to involve attempts to manage the way in which stressful events are perceived. Looking for personal or professional help is an example of Approach caping mechanisms. In contrast, Avoidance coping mechanisms are disengagement or passive responses in attempt to withdraw without steps to change or solve stressful situations. An example of passive coping is day dreaming. Individuals who use these type of responses perceive stressful events as being uncontrollable (Moos, Rudolf H., 1993). Statement of £2ng The educational process of becoming a physician involves a series of stressful events students must deal with as they progress through medical school. While many of the stressful events that medical students experience are the result of social pressure and the maturation process common to all young adults, some are unique to the process of medical education. It is well known that all medical students face stressful circumstances during the course of their education, and that their success in dealing with stress is dependent upon how well they can adjust to a continually changing and demanding 08:8 IRES-I: imp-o and a iatlgu COmn beam: environment. At most medical schools, issues of increased stress appears to come about and interfere with the student’s progress through the four year curriculum. First year students muSt deal with stress involving their sense of competency. They find that not only is the course material complex, placing increased intellectual demand on them, but also that competition is keen and they may no longer be earning honor grades as they once did as undergraduates. Exacerbating competency concerns, first year students must master a vast quantity of material in a limited time period and must learn to set self- imposed limits on the amount of material which can be learned. They also must come to recognize the sacrifices a career in medicine will require and must make personal and social adjustments (Elam, 1994). First and second year students receive little personal formative feedback from faculty and no immediate reward for their hard work. They also note an increased level of fatigue stemming from continuous academic pressures. Often, they may question their commitment to medicine, lose some early idealism, and question their motives for _ becoming a physician (Gaensbauer and Mizner, 1980). According to Mosley, et al. (1994), it has been found that medical education is an extremely intense and stressful experience, and that the continuous demanding and competitive environment students face often exerts a negative effect on their academic performance, physical health, and psychological well-being. Numerous common academic frustrations can precipitate a stress reaction in medical students. The following are just few examples of issues which can contribute to stress reactions: having to master large amounts of difficult material, fearing failure on final M5N~ . sis ..\.v hat examinations, receiving limited positive feedback, having problems in interaction with other fellow students (peer-group study), and having to manage diverse demands during extremely long study hours both inside and outside the classroom. Beyond curricular and personal oriented stresses, medical students also face maturational stressors. Personal development issues include relations with a spouse or significant other, childbearing and child care, physical health and wellness, finances, housing concerns, and lack of leisure time. The complexity of stressful situations from curricular and/ or personal events, can have an impact upon medical school performance. Medical students may react to academic frustrations with anxiety responses which may either enhance or impede their actions. Debilitating anxiety may manifest itself in dysfunctional behaviors; for example, students may stop attending classes, resort to ineffective study techniques such as spending hours listening to tape lectures, become cynical, and, more destructively, may resort to cheating, drug or alcohol abuse, and suicidal thinking. The net result of such dysfunctional behaviors is decreased self-esteem, depression, anxiety, loneliness and alienation, and failure to perform to their capability (Westo and Peterson, 1980). The purpose of this study is to investigate whether the use of selected (Approach and/ or Avoidance) coping mechanisms by first and second year medical students, is related to their academic performance. With this study, the researcher will examine coping strategies medical students use while going through considerable stress. Participants will be first and second year medical students who are enrolled and attending classes at the College of Human Medicine, Michigan State University during Fall of 1997. Si ' 1 cc f Researc Stress among medical students has been linked to poor academic performance, while supportive social relationships have been associated with the alleviation of psychological stress. This study will examine students’ coping skills as potential buffers against stress. The study will also attempt to raise awareness about the need for greater attention to the psychological well-being of medical students; in particular those students in the first and second preclinical years. Mien; This study will be limited by: 1. The sample subjects will be students from one medical program. 2. The extent to which the selected inventory measures all stress related situations, and the impact of stress on each student’s life over the course of the study. 3. The inability to control for past experiences of each student in dealing with stress, and the uniqueness of their impact. 4. The accuracy of the description of the source of stress that students will report. 5. The accuracy of the student’s reports regarding their academic standing, i.e., good academic standing, academic difficulties and/ or on academic probation. c ion 1. Is there variation in students’ selected (Approach and/ or Avoidance) coping strategies and their academic performance 2. Is there variation in students’ selected (Approach and/ or Avoidance) coping mechanisms when they are grouped by gender? 3. Is there variation in students’ selected (Approach and/ or Avoidance) coping mechanisms when they are grouped by age? 4. Is there variation in students’ selected (Approach and/ or Avoidance) coping mechanisms when they are grouped by marital status? 5. Is there variation in students’ selected (Approach and/ or Avoidance) coping mechanisms when they are grouped by ethnic background? 6. Is there variation in students’ selected (Approach and/ or Avoidance) coping mechanisms when they are grouped by preclinical year. Hm To respond to the research questions, the following hypothesis will be tested: Hypothesis 1: There is a positive relationship between the students’ use of selected (Approach and/ or Avoidance) coping strategies and their academic performgce, Null: There is no relationship between the students’ use of selected (Approach and/ or Avoidance) caping strategies and their academic performance. Hypothesis 2: There is a relationship between the students’ use of (Approach and/ or Avoidance) selected coping mechanisms and their gender, Null: There is no relationship between the students’ use of selected (Approach and/ or Avoidance) coping mechanisms and their gender. Hypothesis 3: There is a relationship between the students’ use of selected (Approach and/ or Avoidance) coping mechanisms and their age, 10 Null: There is no relationship between the students’ use of selected (Approach and/ or Avoidance) coping mechanisms and their age. Hypothesis 4: There is a relationship between the students’ use of selected (Approach and/ or Avoidance) coping mechanisms and their marital status. Null: There is no relationship between the students’ use of selected (Approach and/ or Avoidance) coping mechanisms and their marital status. Hypothesis 5: There is a relationship between the students’ use of selected (Approach and/ or Avoidance) coping mechanisms and their ethnic background. Null: There is no relationship between the students’ use of selected (Approach and/ or Avoidance) coping mechanisms and their ethnic background. Hypothesis 6: There is a relationship between the students’ use of selected (Approach and/ or Avoidance) coping mechanisms and their year in the program. Null: There is no relationship between the students’ use of selected (Approach and/ or Avoidance) coping mechanisms and their year in the program. Definition of Terminology Stress: The response of the body to any demands or pressure. It is the force that precipitates the disruption of a person’s normal sense of well-being. Stress also refers to any factor that threatens the health of the body or has adverse effects on its functioning, such as injury, disease or worry. The existence of one form of stress tends to diminish resistance to other forms. (The Bantam Medical Dictionary, 1990) Coping Skills/Reponses: Coping refers to efforts to master conditions or harm, threat or challenge. Lazarus defines coping responses as “those direct active tendencies aimed at 11 eliminating or minimizing a stressful event which are task and reality oriented.” (Lazanrs,1997). Social Support: It is help we receive from friends, relatives, colleagues and mentors. Early definitions referred to an emotional caring dimension. For example, Moss (1973) defined support as “the subjective feeling of belonging, of being accepted or being loved, of being needed all for oneself and for what one can do.” Approach Coping Responses: In general, approach coping is problem-focused; it reflects cognitive and behavioral efforts to master or resolve life stressors. Avoidance Coping Responses: Avoidance coping tends to be emotion-focused; it reflects cognitive and behavioral attempts to avoid thinking about a stressor and its implications, or to manage the effect associated with. Academic Performance: For the purpose of this study, academic performance will be defined as the students’ academic status (i.e., in good standing, on academic probation, failing courses at the time they complete the study’s inventory). Organizatign 9f the Study For the purpose of convenience and methodical procedure, this study will be organized in five chapters. Chapter I is the introduction to the study and background, a statement and purpose of the study, the significance of the research and the limitations of the project. This chapter will also address the research questions, hypothesis and the definition of terminology. Chapter II is a review of the literature, focusing on research involving medical students and their stress coping mechanisms. 12 Chapter III is an introduction and description of the methodology, study’s population sample, the process of survey, description of the survey instrument and the analysis of data. Chapter IV is a detailed discussion of the findings. Chapter V is the summary and recommendations. 13 CHAPTER 11 REVIEW OF RELATED LITERATURE This chapter does not purport to be an exhaustive review of the literature available on the general topic of stress coping mechanisms. Instead, it is intended to be a brief historical review on the subject of stress coping mechanisms, and in more detail, the literature pertinent to this study. These include a review of the following: (a) the General Adaptation Theory, (b) the Cognitive Appraisal Model, (d) the Transactional Model and (e) the main sources of stress experienced by medical students and their effect on academic performance. The General Adaptation Theogy Hans Selye (1956) was one of the first who tried to explain the process of stress- related illness. His “General Adaptation Syndrome” theory consists of three stages that individuals encounter in dealing with” stressful situations: 1. The alarm reaction, in which an initial shock phase of lowered physiological/ immune resistance is followed by countershock during which the individual’s defense mechanisms become active. 14 1Tb: ICIL' wet" I 3. It". seen In: consider situation consequ more or on the i M Tl €Xplair and Fr In the being rein-a and 1: the In Verdi hOW I: 2. The stage of maximum adaptation which if successfully accomplished, the individual returns to an equilibrium. However, if the stressor continues or the defense does not work, he will encounter the third stage. 3. Exhaustion stage which results when the adaptive mechanisms collapse. This stage is seen as the precursor to a host of somatic and emotional illness. In spite of its popularity, Selye’s theory has been criticized for failing to take under consideration individual differences in relation to how people deal with stressful situations (e.g. some people thrive in extremely stressful jobs, without negative health consequences). Nevertheless, Selye’s theory initiated research efforts that have focused more on the psychological aspects of stress, such as the nature of the stress and its effect on the individual’s behavior. The Cognitive Appraisfl The Cognitive Appraisal Model is one of the psychological models that seeks to explain stress and individual differences in people’s reaction to it. According to Lazarus and Folkman’s model (1984) appraisal is composed of primary and secondary processes. In the primary appraisal, the person may ask him or herself how relevant to his/ her well being a situation may be. This represents a basic risk assessment. If the encounter is relevant to the person’s well being, he or she may judge the situation (in terms of Lazarus and Folkrnan’s model, 1984) as a challenge, threat or harm. With the secondary appraisal, the individual is concerned with what can he or she do to resolve the situation. In other I words, during the secondary appraisal, the individual makes a conscious decision about how to deal with the stressful event. The decision-making process must take under 15 conside coping , tron: smmm particul. appraisa contriht ° thee 'thh ‘ tl'ICa ' then The mhnh “ample. Person at other h “I how we“ W consideration the individual’s coping resources (e.g. social support), his or her preferred coping style (e.g. Approach or Avoidance coping mechanisms), the options available to the person and the nature of the situation (Cox; 1987). The Tmactipnal‘ Model Cox, (1985), Cox and Mackay, (1981), Ferguson and Cox, (1991), discuss their model as a primary appraisal process that takes into account a number of different personal and situational factors. Each person, according to his/ her different experiences, judges a particular situation as challenging, anxiety-producing or depressing. The last two appraisals are the only ones experienced as negatively stressful. The four factors that contribute to the appraisal process are: 0 the external and internal demands that the person experiences, 0 their personal coping abilities and resources, 0 the amount or degree of control they have over coping, and 0 the support that they received from others in coping with stressful situations. The transactional model has given more importance to the concept of control, since under this model control, contributes to both primary and secondary appraisals. For example, when a person is faced with a situation, the primary appraisal occurs when the person asks him or herself if the situation represents a problem to him or her. On the other hand, a secondary appraisal occurs when the person asks him/ herself, how and how well they can cope with the problem, (Cox, 1987). Therefore, in the transactional model, primary appraisal is seen as a continual process or monitoring process while the 16 secondary appraisal is seen as an activity that involves decision making contingent upon the primary appraisal. According to Cooper and Payne (1991), primary appraisal is a subject of mediation dictated by individual differences. Individual differences may exist in relation to the person’s perception of the situation as being negatively stressful or not. For example, a negatively stressful situation is usually accompanied by a person’s negative feelings or emotions such as anxiety or depression. These emotions could vary in intensity according to each individual, and they could also indicate the level of stress experienced by each individual. (Cox, 1985,1990). According to Bandura (1977), an eminent Social. Learning theorist, people vary in their ability to cope with demands, and in their perceptions of those abilities. Such variations may be a function of their intelligence, their experience and education, or their beliefs in their ability to cope. As Bandura, Cooper and Payne (1991) also believe that people may vary in the amount of control they can exercise over any situation but not only as a function of that situation but also as a function of their beliefs about control. Additionally, people may vary in their need for social support, the skills that they have in utilizing that support and their perception of that support. The Main Spurces of Stress Exgrienced by Medical Students and Their Effe_ct on Academic Performance. Even though numerous studies have documented the stress that medical students experience, the literature related to stress coping mechanisms and how they may affect the academic performance of medical students is sparse. Also, no study on this subject utilizing the CRI-Adult inventory has appeared in the literature. Therefore, only the most relevant literature sources related to the subject of this study are considered in this section. In their book, Making It In Medical School, Coombs and St. John (1979), refer to freshman medical students’ source of stress as falling into two broad categories: actual and anticipated. Some examples of actual sources of stress are those related to the workload and the increased pressure to study constantly. The fear of academic failure is an anticipated source of stress. Some of the other stressors that these authors mention have to do with status loss, unfamiliar academic pressures, financial constrains and social isolation. They also report that 20-30% of medical students seek psychological assistance, while some 40% acknowledge considering dropping out during the first two years. Early studies on medical student stress such as the Gaensbauer and Mizner (1980), hypothesize that “students’ emotional problems derive as much from the nature of developmental stresses they must face as from their own individual vulnerability and that to study this developmental stresses might prove fruitful in determining what type of coping strategies or tasks would be most helpful” (page 60). To support their hypothesis, Gaensbauer and Mizner reviewed the files of all students who sought psychiatric consultation at the University of Colorado Medical School over ten years. After reviewing the case files, they “attempted to identify recurring themes which might reflect specific developmental issues which must be dealt with by all medical students” (pages. 66-67). They concentrated on specific developmental tasks that confront students and the ensuing stresses that are likely to occur if they are not successfully managed. 18 Gaensbauer and Mizner’s findings support Boyle and Coomb’s observation (1971), which identifies academic pressure and fear of failure as two of the main stressors for the first year medical student. According to Gaensbauer and Mizner, the initial task for the first year student is “to determine personal capabilities in this new context and to perform in a manner that equals one’s ability, while maintaining sense of adequacy” (pages 57-68). They also suggest that failure to develop such coping strategies could likely result in the student’s decreased self-esteem, depression and anxiety. Additional results could be the students’ withdrawal from competition and their inability to perform academically at their best level. Another major source of stress for first year medical students is the load or vast amount of information that needs to be learned within a short period of time. This is often referred to as “load”. In addition to the academic demands, a third source of stress for first year medical students is the lack of time for personal and social relationships. Due to the demands on the students’ time, the tasks of reestablishing and building new social relationships may become difficult. A common mistake that first year students make is to give to academics their exclusive attention, to the neglect of their personal and emotional needs. The result is often a feeling of dehumanization deriving from this “tunnel vision”. It occurs when students’ excessive academic demands preclude time for any pursuit of personal needs or interests. According to Elam (1994), all medical students face stressful circumstances during the course of their medical education. Success in dealing with stress is dependent upon how well they can adjust to a changing and demanding environment. At most medical schools, issues of developmental stress appear along the lines of the four-year curriculum. First 19 year students must deal with stresses involving their sense of competency . They find that not only is the course material complex, placing increase intellectual demands on them, but also that competition is keen and they may no longer be earning honors grades as they once had as undergraduates. Exacerbating competency concerns, first year students must master a vast quantity of material in a limited time period and they must learn to set self- imposed limits on the amount of material which can be learned. They also must come to recognize the sacrifices a career in medicine will require and must make personal and social adjustments in response. Some of developmental stressors that medical students encounter in the third and fourth years have to do with their transition from the lecture hall to the hospitals where they directly participate in patient care during the last two years of medical school. Throughout this time, students continue to participate in regular lectures with attending physicians, residents and other allied health professionals. All together depending on the rotation, the students spend between ten and fourteen hours per day in the hospital and generally take overnight calls every third or fourth day. Besides curricular and medically oriented stresses, Elam mentions other developmental issues that medical students face. Those are: relationships with spouse or significant other, childbearing and child care, physical health and wellness, finances, housing concerns, and lack of leisure time. She asserts that the complexity of developmental stress—either from curricular or personal events, or interaction between the two—can have an impact upon the student’s medical school performance. Thomas H. Mosley, et al. (1994) reviewed the effects of the medical school environment on the academic performance, physical health, and psychological well-being 20 of 69 third year students. The results of the study showed that 23% of the students suffered clinical depression and 57% endorsed high levels of somatic distress. Stress accounted for a large percentage of distress variance (i.e., 29% to 50%), and caping efforts contributed significant variance to the prediction of distress. Coping efforts classified by Engagement (Approach) strategies were associated with fewer depressive symptoms, while Disengagement (Avoidance) strategies were associated with higher levels of depressive symptoms. These results suggest that training in Engagement strategies may be a useful intervention to lessen the negative consequences of stress among medical students. In terms of social support, it is well known that the heavy work load and uncertainty become more difficult to manage when medical students are unable to find strength in a group of close friends. Students often complain about the difficulties that they have in developing a new support network or just making friends and socializing, which can be frustrating due also to the lack of free time. In addition, family and close friends may have difficulty in understanding why medical students must dedicate most of their time studying, thus creating more stress. Rospenda, Halpert and Richman’s study (1994), they examined social support as a defense against stress and hence as a potential strengthener of student’s academic performance of 112 third year medical students. After assessing role stress (stress involving compelling demands between school, social and family life), social support, and sources of support (outside or inside medical school), their findings reveal that no buffering effect was found for social support. In fact, social support from outside medical school exhibits significant variance in academic performance and the students’ levels of stress. For 21 instance, higher levels of outside support were associated with poorer clerkship grades for women, but with lower levels of stress for men. Contrary to the study hypothesis, social support in general was related to lower levels of academic performance for both men and women. Stewart, et al. (1995), conducted a study on stress and vulnerability in medical students. They gave a survey to 140 Hong Kong Chinese students in their second year of their medical education, and compared them with 138 students who completed the same survey prior to the beginning of their first year of medical school, and 74 non-medical university students in their second year. The relevant findings of the research were the following: there was a loss of opportunity to maintain social and recreational sources of gratification correlated with increased anxiety. There was no difference between the sexes with regard to the development of anxiety and depression symptoms. Academically less successful students reported somewhat higher levels of depressive ideation and symptomatology. Trait anxiety (chronically experienced anxiety, as opposed to situational anxiety) correlated with the development of distress. Active coping styles (Approach) and positive reinterpretation (Positive Appraisal) as coping strategies correlated negatively with distress, while wishful thinking (Avoidance) correlated positively with distress. Finally, one of the most recent considerations regarding medical students stress is that of John A. Toews, et al. (1997). This study based on the self-reporting of 1,681 medical students, medical residency program trainees, and graduate science students from four Canadian schools of medicine. The study results showed few significant differences between the respondents at the four schools. For example, in terms of the medical 22 students and medical residency program trainees, it was found that the main concerns of the respondents in these groups were the volume of their work and the limited time available for learning. Among other findings, while all groups reported having support from friends and spouses/ partners, medical residency program trainees received more support from these sources than the other groups. In terms of gender, the study mentions some differences related to women reporting higher levels of stress than men, and also women as more likely than men to report concerns about the volume and complexity of the learning material. The study supports the notion that, for medical students, stressors include the volume of material to be learned, academic performance anxiety and evaluations. The researchers assert that while the external demands of a situation affect the response to it, a person’s assessment of his or her own coping skills, and the ability to negotiate the situation also significantly affects the response. 23 CHAPTER III RESEARCH METHODOLOGY The purpose guiding this study is to investigate if the use of selected (Approach and/ or Avoidance) coping strategies and by preclinical (first and second year) medical students, is related to their academic performance . The study will focus on two basic types of coping strategies: approach versus avoidance. The efficacy of these strategies when applied to academic and non-academic sources of stress in medical students will be examined. Quantitative methodology will be used in this study. Due to its ability to identify attributes of the sample population (first and second year medical students); the economy of its design; the rapid turn around in data collection; and convenience, the COping Response Inventory (CRI-Adult) is the preferred type of data collection instrument for this study. The nature of the inventory is cross sectional, and it will be administered to groups of forty to thirty Students at a single time. Research Qpegipm 1. Is there variation in students’ selected (Approach and/ or Avoidance) coping strategies and their academic performance 2. Is there variation in students’ selected (Approach and/ or Avoidance) coping mechanisms when they are grouped by gender? 24 3. Is there variation in students’ selected (Approach and/ or Avoidance) coping mechanisms when they are grouped by age? 4. Is there variation in students’ selected (Approach and/ or Avoidance) coping mechanisms when they are grouped by marital status? 5. Is there variation in students’ selected (Approach and/ or Avoidance) coping mechanisms when they are grouped by ethnic background? 6. Is there variation in students’ selected (Approach and/ or Avoidance) coping mechanisms when they are grouped by preclinical year. meat: To respond to the research questions, the following hypothesis will be tested: Hypothesis 1: There is a positive relationship between the students’ use of selected (Approach and/ or Avoidance) coping strategies and their academic mgfprmance. Null: There is no relationship between the students’ use of selected (Approach and/ or Avoidance) coping strategies and their academic performance. Hypothesis 2: There is a relationship between the students’ use of selected (Approach and/ or Avoidance) coping mechanisms and their gm; Null: There is no relationship between the students’ use of selected (Approach and/ or Avoidance) coping mechanisms and their gender. Hypothesis 3: There is a relationship between the students’ use of selected (Approach and/ or Avoidance) coping mechanisms and their age Null: There is no relationship between the students’ use of selected (Approach and/ or Avoidance) coping mechanisms and their age. 25 Hypothesis 4: There is a relationship between the students’ use of selected (Approach and/ or Avoidance) coping mechanisms and their marital status. Null: There is no relationship between the students’ use of selected (Approach and/ or Avoidance) coping mechanisms and their marital status. Hypothesis 5: There is a relationship between the students’ use of selected (Approach and/ or Avoidance) coping mechanisms and their ethpic background. Null: There is no relationship between the students’ use of selected (Approach and/ or Avoidance) coping mechanisms and their ethnic background. Hypothesis 6: There is a relationship between the students’ use of selected (Approach and/ or Avoidance) coping mechanisms and their year in the program. Null: There is no relationship between the students’ use of selected (Approach and/ or Avoidance) coping mechanisms and their year in the program. Population and Sample The population of the study will be comprised of all medical students in the first and second years of the College of Human Medicine at Michigan State University. This school has at least 110 students in each class or academic year. The total population for the first and second year of this particular program is comprised by 205 students, of these, 106 are males and 99 are females. The youngest student is 23 years old, with the oldest being 44 years of age. To gain access to this population, the researcher will contact the office of student affairs at the medical program (see Appendix A, page 53). To assure representativeness of sample, all students in the first and second year class attending the selected medical school, will be invited to participate in completing the 26 Coping Response Adult Inventory (CRI- Adult). Since the researcher will have access to all students in the two academic levels or years, a single stage sampling procedure will be used. The selection of individuals for this study will be based on each student’s willingness to participate. No less than 70 students will constitute the sample for this study. Infomed Consent Prior to the administration of the CRI-Adult Inventory, the students will be asked to give their informed consent to use the information given on the Inventory for the research by signing a letter of consent, (see Appendix B, page 54). Data Collegion The CRI—Adult Inventory will be administered to groups of thirty to forty medical students at a time on a designated date and room at the selected school. Consideration in timing will be given to accommodate the students' schedule. The researcher will have to relay on the accuracy of the students’ reports regarding their academic standing. Instrumentation Contemporary theories emphasize the multidimensional aspects of appraisal and coping processes. In brief, researchers have used two main conceptual approaches to classifycoping responses. One approach emphasizes the orientation or focus of coping (problem-focused or emotion focused), whereas the other emphasizes the method of coping (cognitive or behavioral), (e.g., Billings 8t Moos, 1981, 1984; Folkman 8L Lazarus, 1985; Lazarus 8c Folkman, 1984; Roth 8: Cohen, 1986). The Coping Responses 27 Inventory-Adult Form (CRI-Adult) combines these two approaches. It assesses eight types of coping responses that reflect these focus-and methods of coping domains. The inventory considers the orientation or focus of coping and separates coping responses into approach and avoidance responses. Each of these two sets of coping responses is divided into two categories that reflect cognitive or behavioral coping methods. In general, approach coping is problem-focused; it reflects cognitive and behavioral efforts to master or resolve life stressors. In contrast, avoidance coping tends to be emotion- focused; it reflects cognitive and behavioral attempts to avoid thinking about a stressor and its implications, or to manage the affect associated with it. Over 100 studies have been conducted on the reliability and validity of the CRI-Adult. Developed and designed by Rudolf H. Moos. In 1981, the CRI-Adult was a 19-item inventory which was originally used in studies of alcoholic patients (Billing 8C Moos, 1981). Later on, a revised 32-item version was utilized in studies done on depressed patients (Billing 8: Moos, 1984). In these earlier studies, researchers primarily focused on findings based on indices of coping that are conceptually and empirically comparable to the scales in the current version of the CRI-Adult. A new 72-item version was administered to a group of more than 1,800 adults, some of whom had drinking problems. Overall, the group included more than 1,100 men and 700 women. The subjects in the study were asked to identify a recent stressful event and to rate their reliance on each of the coping items on a four-point scale. Analysis of the data from this field trial led to the current 48-item version of the CRI-Adult which is composed of eight scales that reflect approach and avoidance coping. The eight scales are shown in Table 1. 28 Table 1 Cfl-Adult Mes Approach coping resmnses Cognitive 1. Logical Analysis 2. Positive Reappraisal Avoidance coping responses 5. Cognitive Avoidance 6. Acceptance or Resignation Behavioral 3. Seeking Guidance 86 Support 7. Seeking Alternative Rewards 4. Problem Solving 8. Emotional Discharge According to Rudolf Moos, (CRI-Adult, Professional Manual, page 14) to reduce redundancy and shorten the inventory, he combined dimension that were conceptually similar and highly intercorrelated. This resulted in the eight scales described in Table 2 Table 2 CRI—Adult Seges an_d Descriptions Scales Descriptions Approach Coping Responses 1. Logical Analysis 2. Positive Reappraisal 3. Seeking Guidance and Support 4. Problem Solving Avoidance Coping Responses 5. Cognitive Avoidance 6. Acceptance or Resignation 7. Seeking Alternative Rewards 8. Emotional Discharge Cognitive attempts to understand and prepare mentally for stressor and its consequences. Cognitive attempts to construe and restructure a problem in a positive way while still accepting the reality of the situation. Behavioral attempts to seek information, guidance or support. Behavioral attempts to take action to deal directly with the problem. Cognitive attempts to avoid thinking realistically about a problem. Cognitive attempts to react to the problem by accepting it. Behavioral attempts to get involved in substitute activities and create new sources of satisfaction. Behavioral attempts to reduce tension by expressing negative feelings 29 eli ili More than 90% of the respondents in the final field trial participated in a 12-month follow-up in which they again complete the CRI-Adult. In general, the coping indices were moderately stable over time among men and women (average rs- .45 and .43, respectively, for the eight indices). Positive Reappraisal, Seeking Guidance and Support, Cognitive Avoidance, and Emotional Discharge were somewhat more stable (average rs- .49 and .47 for men and women respectively) than Logical Analysis and Problem Solving (average rs- .41 and .39 for men and women respectively) these stability’s are comparable to those found in a study done over 1 to 2 year interval among alcoholic and depressed patients and normal controls. (Billings 8C Moos, 1985a; Fondacaro 85 Moos, 1987; Holahan 8L Moos,1987a). Individual propensities toward approach and avoidance coping may remain moderately stable over longer intervals. For example, in a study done by Swindle, Cronkite 8C Moos, 1989, a 3 year stability coefficients of between .34 and .48 for Seeking Guidance and Support, Problem Solving, and Emotional Discharge coping among depressed patients was found. Among alcoholic patients and their spouses, it was also found somewhat lower 8-year stability coefficients averaged between .13 and .38 for indices of cognitive approach, behavioral-approach, and avoidance coping. Avoidance coping was the most stable (rs- .38 and .30 for patients and spouses, respectively). Thus, there is some consistency over time in individuals' coping responses despite the variety of stressful circumstances they encounter. 30 Qggm' iog pf the Inventory The front (top sheet) of the CRI-Adult answer sheet contains areas for recording basic demographic information and the description of the problem or situation. (see Appendix C, page 55). The description of the problems or situations the students will respond will be categorized as follows: Academic Performance Load or Academic Load Adjustment to medical school Family Health Social Relationships Financial Multiple (more than one of the above) Other (Unspecified) PPNP‘PH‘P’NE“ A template for scoring the scales of the inventory is also provided. Part 1 of the Inventory, includes 10 stressor-appraisal items which are not scored but provide important information about how the individual perceives the stressor. Part 2 of the Inventory, includes forty eight items that relate to both approach and avoidance coping responses, (see Appendix C, pages 58,59). A four point scale is used to rate students’ reliance on each coping item. Also, a profile form which allows the conversion of raw scores to T scores is included, (see Appendix D, page 60). The CRI-Adult can be hand-scored by using the provided scoring template. A profile area is provided on the reverse side of the answer sheet. This profile allows the researcher to convert the subject's raw scores to T scores (M- 50; SD- 10) and to plot the 31 respondent’s coping responses profile. The coping response profile is based on two types of coping responses, those are: approach coping responses and avoidance coping. The profile will be marked with horizontal lines to facilitate interpretation of scores. A dotted horizontal line marks the mean of the T-score distribution. The higher the T—score, the more the student utilizes a specific coping response. The lower the T-score, the less the student utilizes a specific coping response. The T-score interpretation is shown in Table 3. Table 3 Criteria for Integpreting CRI-Adult Standard Scores T- score range Equivalent percentile range Description 234 _>_ 6 Considerably below average 35 - 40 7 - 16 Well below average 41 - 45 17 - 33 Somewhat below Average 46 - 54 34 - 66 Average 55 - 59 67 - 83 Somewhat above average 60 -65 84 - 93 Well above average 366 394 Considerably above average Academic Performance The information data about the academic performance of students will be gathered through the student’s own volunteered response to the description of the problem or situation section on the CRI—Adult answering sheet. The student will have the opportunity to explain if the nature of his/ her problem was due to his/ her academic difficulties, e.g., failing to pass a course or more, and being on academic probation. 32 Data Analysis An attempt will be made to find if there is a relationship between the students’ coping response and their academic difficulties. The approach coping response identified as Seeking Guidance (SG) will be used to measure the students’ social support. After obtaining students’ CRI—Adult T-scores, these will be used for statistical analysis, an the researcher will compare results among individuals according to the following independent variables: gender, age, marital status, ethnic background and pre- clinical year. This will be done by obtaining the means and standard deviation for each one of the variables already mentioned. To measure the relationship between the selected (Approach and/ or Avoidance) coping responses, the Pearson Correlation Coefficient will be used. Null hypothesis will be tested at a significant level of p > .05, using the Statistical package for Social Sciences (SPSS). To test the independent variable (Academic Performance), the Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) will be used. The purpose of the ANOVA is to test whether the difference or variance among the means of the two samples is significant or can be attributed to change. To examine the main and interactive effects of coping mechanisms and social support on academic performance, multiple regressions will be performed between the two samples. 33 CHAPTER I V DISCUSSION OF FINDINGS The Coping Response Inventory for Adult (CRI-Adult Inventory) which consists of two parts. Part 1 has 10 questions that assess the students’ sources of stress. These questions are not scored but provide important information about how the student perceives the stressor(s). The sources of stress are categorized as follows : academic performance, academic or workload, adjustment to medical school, family, health, social relationships, financial, multiple and other. In Part 2, which includes 48 items, the students are asked to indicate how often they engaged in a particular behavior in connection with the problem or source of stress they described in Part 1. Each of the 48 items measures either an Approach or an Avoidance coping response. The Approach coping responses are defined as follows: LA or logical analysis, PR or positive reappraisal, SG or seeking guidance and support, and PS or problem solving. The Avoidance coping responses are defined as follows: CA or cognitive avoidance, AR or acceptance and resignation, SR or seeing alternative rewards, and ED or emotional discharge. A four-point scale was used to rate the students’ reliance on each coping item, (see Appendix C, page 57 for CRI-Adult Inventory). 34 Results of the CRI—Adult Inventog The results of the administration of the CRI-Adult Inventory to the students can be seen by the following tables: Table 1 Number and Percent of Students Responding to the CRI-Adult Inventog: N - 73 Year in School Number % 1 47 64.5 2 26 35.5 Total 73 100% Eli—dc; Male 35 48.0 Female 38 52.0 Total 73 100% m 21-25 39 54.0 26-30 24 33.0 31-35 4 5 .5 36-40 5 6.5 4144 1 1.0 Total 73 100% MEL—Mus. Single 53 73.0 Married 19 26.0 Divorced 1 1.0 Total 73 100% 35 Table 2 Frequency of Types of Strgssor Repgrted by Medical Students: N-73 Stressor # of Students Responding % Academic 11 15.0" Load 14 19.3" Adjustment 7 9.7 Social 11 15.0" Family 6 8.2 Health 4 5.5 Finances 4 5.5 Multiple 10 13.6 Other 6 8.2 Total 73 100% 1. Academic - academic performance, i.e., failing courses, on academic probation. 2. Load - workload or amount of material to be learned. 3. Adjustment - transition into medical school. 4. Social - social relationships, i.e., friendships and dating. 5. Family - family relationships and marriage difficulties. 6. Health - own or family. 7. Finances - financial difficulties or constraints. 8. Multiple - having more than one type of stressor. 36 9.Unspecified - student selected nm to specify the source of stress. * Most frequently reported stressors were related to load, academic difficulties and social relationships. Table 3 Fregpeng; pf Types of Stressor Reported by Gender: N-73 km; W Male % Female % Total Academic 4 11.4 7 "* 18.4 11 Load 9 ”25.7 5 13.1 14 Adjust 4 11.4 3 g 8 7 Social 6 17 5 13.1 1 1 Family 2 5.7 4 10.5 6 Health 1 2.8 3 8 4 Finances 3 8.5 1 2.6 4 Multiple 3 8.6 7 I"”184 10 Unspecified 3 8.6 3 8 6 Total 35 100 38 100 73 The most frequent type of stressors reported by gender were as follows: *Workload or academic load, with males expressing more concerns related to the workload than females. *"’Academic standing or difficulties, with females reporting as having more difficulties than males. 37 *“Multiple, with a high percentage of females reporting this as their main stressor. ****More females than males reported having multiple sources of stress. Table 4 Frequeng pf Types of Stressors Reported by Age Group: N-73 Stressor Age Group 21-25 °/o 26-30 % 3 1-3 5 % 36-40 % 41-44 % Total Academic 1 2.6 9 I"‘36 1 16.6 11 Load 9 23.6 3 12 2 33.4 14 Adjust 4 10.5 2 8 1 16.6 7 Social 10 26.3 1 4 1 1 Family 1 2.6 3 12 2 67 6 Health 3 8 1 4 4 Finances 2 5.2 1 16.6 1 100 4 Multiple 5 13.1 3 12 1 16.6 1 33 10 Unspecified 3 8 3 12 6 Total 38 100 25 100 6 100 3 100 1 100 73 The most frequent type of stressors reported by age were as follows: * Load, with students ages 21 to 25 years old reporting more workload type of stressor than the older the students. *"’Academic, with students ages 26 to 30 years old reporting more academic difficulties than the rest of the students. “*Social, with students ages 21 to 25 years old reporting more social stressors than the older students. Table 5 Erggugpcy pf Types of Stressgr Repgrted by Marital Status: N -73 Stressor Marital Status Single °/o Married % Divorced % Total Academic 8 15 3 15 1 1 Load 7 13.2 7 "’36 14 Adjust 7 13.2 7 Social 11 "21 1 1 Family 2 3.7 4 20 6 Health 3 5.6 1 5 4 Finances 2 3.7 2 10 1 100 4 Multiple 8 15 2 10 10 Unspecified 5 9.4 1 5 6 Total 53 100 19 100 1 100 73 The most frequent type of stressor reported by marital status were as follows: *Workload or academic load, with equal number of single and married students expressing workload as their main stressor. “Academic standing or performance, with single students expressing more academic difficulties than married students. “*Social relationships, with single students reporting social as their main stressor. 39 ““Multiple, with more single students reporting multiple sources of stress than married Students. Table 6 Frequency of Types of Stressor Reported by Year in School: N-73 Spam Year Years 1 8L 2 3/9 1 % 2 % Combined Academic 7 14.7 4 15.3 11 15 Load 7 14.7 7 *26.9 14 "'”"’19 Adjust 6 12.7 1 3.8 7 9.6 Social 9 “19.1 2 7.5 11 15 Family 4 8.5 7.5 6 8.2 Health 2 4.2 7.5 4 5.5 Finances 3 6.3 3.8 4 5.5 Multiple 6 12.7 15.3 10 14 Unspecified 3 6.3 3 1 1.5 6 8.2 Total 47 100 26 100 73 100% The most frequent type of stressor reported by first and second year students were: *Workload or academic load, with equal number of first and second year students reporting workload as their main stressor. “Academic standing or performance, with more first year students reporting academic difficulties than second year students. 40 M"Social relationships, with more first year students reporting social relationships type of stressor than second year students. The Pearson Correlation Sample coefficient Analysis was applied to both Approach and Avoidance coping responses to examine how they, as test variables, were related to each other, (see Appendix E, Table 7, page 65 for The Pearson Correlation). The results of the Pearson Correlation Coefficient showed no relationship among the test variables. Therefore, an Independent Sample T-Test and the Levene’s Test for Equality of Means procedures, at a significant level of P > .05 were used to compare means among the test variables (Approach and Avoidance coping responses) and the grouping variables (gender, age, marital status, academic performance, workload, social, etc.) Testing of Hypothesis On the basis of the T-test and the Levene’s Test for Equality of Variance, the researcher reports the following findings: Hypothesis 1: There is a positive relationship between the students' use of selected (Approach and/or’Avoidance) coping mechanisms and their academic performance. Null: There is no relationship between the students' use of selected (Approach and/ or Avoidance) coping mechanisms and their academic performance. No statistically significant difference was found between the Approach coping responses (LA, PR, SG and PS) and the students who reported academic difficulties. Therefore, there is no relationship between the student’s academic performance and the use of Approaching coping mechanisms, (see Appendix B, Table 8, page 66). 41 In terms of the Avoidance coping responses (CA, AR, SR and ED), only CA or Cognitive Avoidance showed a statiStically significant difference (F - 3.884, P < 0.53), with students who reported academic difficulties scoring higher in the use of cognitive avoidance type of coping mechanisms. Hypothesis 2: There is a relationship between the students' use of selected (Approach and/ or Avoidance) coping mechanisms and their gelld_er_. Null: There is no relationship between the students' use of selected (Approach and/ or Avoidance) coping mechanisms and their gender. No statistically significant difference was found between the Approach coping responses (LA, PR, SG and PS) and the students’ gender. Therefore, there is no positive relationship between the students’ gender and their use of Approach coping mechanisms, (see Appendix E, Table 9, page 67). In terms of the Avoidance coping responses (CA, AR, SR and ED), only CA or Cognitive Avoidance showed statistically significant difference (F - 8.691, P < .004), with females using more cognitive avoidance type of responses than males. This suggests, that there is a positive relationship between the students’ gender and their use of CA or Cognitive Avoidance coping mechanisms. Some examples of cognitive avoidance type of responses that the students with academic difficulties mentioned they used were: try to forget about the problem or source of stress, try not to think about the problem, daydreaming or imagine a better time or place they were in, put off thinking about the situation, even though they knew they would have to deal with it at some point, they try to deny how serious the problem really was, and they wish the 42 problem would go away or somehow be over with, (see Appendix E, Table 10, page 68). Hypothesis 3: There is a relationship between the students' use of selected (Approach and/ or Avoidance) coping mechanisms and their _age_. Null: There is no relationship between the students' use of selected (Approach an/ or Avoidance) coping mechanisms and their age. No statistically difference was found between the Approach coping responses (LA, PR, SG and PS), and the students’ age. Therefore, there is no positive relationship between the students’ age and their use of Approach coping mechanisms, (see Appendix E, Table 11, page 69). In terms of the Avoidance coping responses (CA, AR, SR and ED), only CA or Cognitive Avoidance showed statistically significant difference (F - 9.138, P < .003) with students ages 21 to 25 years old scoring higher in the use of cognitive avoidance type of coping mechanisms the the Other students. This finding suggests that there is a positive relationship between the students’ ages 21 to 25 years old and their use of CA or Cognitive Avoidance mechanisms, (see Appendix B, Table 11, page 69). No statistically significant difference was found between the Approach coping responses (LA,PR, SG and PS) and students ages 26 to 30 years old. Therefore, there is no positive relationship between students ages 26 to 30 years old and their use of Approach coping responses, (see Appendix E, Table 12, page 70). There is some statistically significant difference (F -3.72, P > .56) between CA or Cognitive Avoidance and students ages 26 to 30 years old. Therefore, there is some 43 positive relationship beWeen students ages 26 to 30 years old and their use of Cognitive Avoidance. Some statistically significant difference was (P -3.020, P > .087) found between the Approach coping response PR or Positive Reappraisal and students ages 36 to 44 years old. Therefore, there is some positive relationship between students ages 36 to 44 years old and their use of positive Reappraisal while undergoing stress, (see Appendix B, Table 13, page 71). Hypothesis 4: There is a relationship between the students' use of selected (Approach and/ or Avoidance) coping mechanisms and their marital status. Null: There is no relationship between the students' use of selected (Approach and/ or Avoidance) caping mechanisms and their marital status. No Statistically significant difference was found between bOth the Approach and the Avoidance c0ping responses (LA, PR, SG, PS, CA, AR, SR and ED), and the students’ marital status. Therefore, there is no positive relationship between the students’ marital status and their use of Approach and / or Avoidance coping responses, (see Appendix B, Table 14, page 72). Hypothesis 5: There is a relationship between the students' use of selected (Approach and/ or Avoidance) coping mechanisms and their minority/non-minority status. Null: There is no relationship between the students' use of selected (Approach and/ or Avoidance) coping mechanisms and their minority/non-minority status. No statistically significant difference was found between both the Approach and the Avoidance coping responses and the minority versus non-minority students. Therefore, there is no positive relationship between the minority versus non-minority 44 students and their use of Approach and/ or Avoidance coping responses, (see Appendix B, Table 15, page 73). Hypothesis 6: There is a relationship between the students' use of selected (Approach and/ or Avoidance) coping mechanisms and their year in the program. Null: There is no relationship between the Students' use of seleCted (Approach and/ or Avoidance) coping mechanisms and their year in the program. Finally, in terms of the students’ use of selected coping responses and their year in the program, no statistically significant difference was found between both the Approach and the Avoidance coping mechanisms (LA, PR, SG, PS, CA, AR, SR and ED) and the students’ year in the program. Therefore, there is no positive relationship between the students’ year in the program and their use of Approach and/ or Avoidance coping mechanisms, (see Appendix E, Table 16, page 74). 45 CHAPTER V SUMMARY AND RECOMMENDATIONS Medical schools have always received a great number of applicants. Selection of students is critical because of the length and the cost of training. Therefore, a considerable amount of attention had been focused on finding accurate and valid predictors of success in medical school. Studies such as Cuca, Sakakeeny and Johnson, (1976) have tried to identify predictors of academic success in medical students. Factors included in these studies were the students’ personality, the Medical College Achievement Test (MCAT) scores, and premedical Grade Point Averages (GPA’s). The studies suggested that there was not clear and definite correlation between these factors and academic success, and that other factors may have intervened. Medical colleges across the nation still make their admission decision based heavily on high MCAT and GPA scores. It is well known that all medical students face stressful circumstances during the course of their education and that their success in dealing with stress is dependent upon how well they can adjust to a continually changing and demanding environment. 46 is: dat Sta: prc 3? ite *1) Perhaps, a more accurate approach to the selection of applicants for medical school is the one that assesses the individual’s ability to deal successfully with highly stressful situations. This study focuses on the medical students’ use of selected (Approach and/ or Avoidance) coping mechanisms and their effect on academic performance. The Coping Response Inventory (CRI-Adult) was the selected instrument for the data collection of the study. First and second year students form the College of Human Medicine at Michigan State University were the selected population sample for this Study. This inventory provides areas for recording the students’ demographic information, the description of a problem or situation they experienced within the past year, and their responses to 48 items related to the specific situation. Each item measures either an Approach and/ or Avoidance response. The approach coping response identified as Seeking Guidance (SG) was used to measure the students’ social support. The problems the students’ described were categorized as follows: academic performance, workload, adjustment to medical school, family relationships, social relationships, health, financial constrains, multiple stressors and other or unspecified. The Pearson Correlation Coefficient Analysis was applied to both Approach and Avoidance coping mechanisms. The results of the Pearson Correlation showed little or no relationship among them. Therefore, an Independent Sample T—Test and the Levene’s Test at a significant level of P > .05 were used to test the study’s hypothesis. 47 Summary of Findings The results of the administration of the CRI-Adult Inventory show that the major stressors for the surveyed medical students were: workload or academic load (19.3% of the students reported workload as the main source of stress) followed by academic difficulties and social relationships as second major stressors (15% of the students reported academic difficulties as major source of stress, and also 15% of the students reported social relationships as major source of stress. With respect to gender differences, the study shows that more men than women reported workload or academic load concerns. More women than men reported having academic difficulties and more women than men reported having multiple sources of stress. In regards to students’ age, the study shows that students ages 21 to 25 years old reported more workload concerns than the rest of the surveyed students. Students ages 26 to 30 years old reported having more academic difficulties than younger or older students did. In terms of the students’ marital status, the study shows that equal number of single and married students expressed workload or academic load concerns. Single students appeared to have more academic difficulties than married students did. Single Students also reported more social relationship difficulties than the married ones. In terms of year in school or first versus second year students, both first and second year students reported workload or academic load as their main source of stress. First year students reported more academic difficulties than second year students did. First 48 year students also reported more social relationship difficulties than second year students did. For example, most of the single students mentioned having difficulties in continuing friendships or breaking engagements or long term relationships. The T-Test and Levene’s Test results Show 1). that the students who reported academic difficulties used more cognitive avoidance mechanisms when compared with students who did not report having academic difficulties, 2). that females in the sample used more cognitive avoidance than males, 3). that students ages 21 to 25 years old used more cognitive avoidance than the older Students, and 4). that students ages 36 to 44 years old used more Positive Reappraisal coping mechanisms than the younger students. Law The study results Show that the students who reported academic difficulties used more cognitive avoidance mechanisms than the other students’ in the sample. These results suggest that, in assessing the personal qualities of applicants to medical school, admissions officers should include some techniques for determining an applicant’s basic strategies in identifying and addressing stressors. This can be done through several means: objective measures (e.g., the CRI-Adult Inventory), and asking applicants to answer problem solving and/ or stress related type of questions on their written essays at the time of their application or during their interviews at the prospective medical schools. While there is no evidence to suggest the use of these techniques as screening devices, the results of these measures could be useful in other ways. 49 Pre—medical and academic advisors of medical students need to raise awareness among their students about the importance of having experience in problem solving, as well as the use of effective coping mechanisms, since they could enhance the students’ well being while in their pre-medical and pre-clinical years. More prevention services are needed to help pre-medical and medical students acquire effective long-term stress reduction skills. Perhaps, classes or lectures as part of the pre-medical, as well as the firSt-year medical curriculum could be introduced in schools and used as training opportunities for students to develop both problem solving and effective coping skills. Other services that can be provided by medical schools are consultation and referral services to students who require psychological type of evaluation and treatment due to extreme stressful experience(s). In order to manage the vast amount of material to be learned in medical schools, pre-medical as well as medical students need to be trained about effective cooperative learning techniques or group study techniques. These are participatory study groups in which each student is in charge of a task or learning material, and each student contributes with a specific information. When the group comes together, each student presents his or her information to the other members of the group. In this way, at the end of each study group meeting, the students not only have learned their independent pieces of information but they have also integrated all relevant pieces of the learning material effectively. 50 Finally, in contrast to the traditional school system of lectures supported by laboratory work that occurs in the pre-clinical years, medical schools could explore more alternatives to the traditional system, such as the “problem based learning system”. Under this approach, the students with faculty guidance present themselves or are presented with case scenarios. The students research and discuss the sample cases, learning the information necessary to diagnose a fictional patient. With this system, the students exercise initiative in directing their education, there is more opportunity for cooperative or team learning, and problem solving skills are developed more towards clinical thinking skills. This system when effectively used could provide the students with training in Approach coping mechanisms such as logical analysis, positive reappraisal, seeking guidance or looking for assistance in gathering accurate information, and problem solving. Most importantly, medical students under the “problem based learning system” are provided with the opportunity to develop effective coping devices that they can apply while under going stress due to school, career and life pressures. More in depth ad longitudinal studies are needed to demonstrate particular trends among medical students; especially those trends that are related to problem solving and the use of coping mechanisms. Future research should also investigate samples from other medical schools, data collected longitudinally through out the program, the effects of the stress intervention efforts on medical school students and comparative studies between pre-clinical and clinical years. 51 APPENDIX A 52 November 17, 1997 Director College of Human Medicine A-254 Life Sciences Building Dear Director: 1 am requesting your consideration and support in conducting a study about the types of coping mechanisms and social support medical students use in dealing with stress, and how these relate to their academic success. This particular study will include students from the College of Human Medicine at Michigan State University. The study will be conducted as a doctoral degree requirement in higher educational administration. Students will be asked to complete the Coping Responses Inventory for Adults. This brief (30 minutes) self-report inventory will identify cognitive and behavioral responses that first and second year medical student used to cope with a recent problem or stressfiil situation. In order to administer the inventory I will carefully make the necessary arrangements (date, location and time), taking under consideration the students’ academic schedule. Individual students are free to participate or not in the study. At any time, a student can decide to withdraw from the study even afier agreeing to participate. There will be no penalty for those students who choose not to participate or to withdraw. Any data collected form of those who decide to withdraw will be destroyed immediately. Numbers will be assigned to students’ responses to ensure the students’ anonymity. Sincerely, Norma Baptista Ph.D Candidate 53 APPENDIX B 54 CONSENT FORM FOR FIRST AND SECOND YEAR MEDICAL STUDENTS I voluntarily agree to participate in the research study, THE USE OF SELECTED COPING STRATEGIES AS PREDICTORS OF ACADEMIC PERFORMANCE, which will be conducted during the 1997/98 academic year. I know that this study is being conducted as dissertation research for Norma Baptista's doctoral studies at Michigan State University. The purpose of this research is to explore the relationship between the use of coping mechanisms as predictors of academic performance in medical students. It is my understanding that my participation in this study involves the following: I I will allow the researcher to administer the research inventory. This will take about one half-hour to one hour of my time. I I will allow the researcher to utilize the information collected through the inventory. I understand that the collected information will be kept in the researchers home in a locked cabinet, to ensure no other person could take or read it. I I realize that although my name will not be used and every effort will be made to keep my identity confidential, it is possible someone at the School could determine my identity. In addition, I understand that: I The data collected through the research inventory will be used in the dissertation, as well as possible articles, presentations or instruction. I The College may or may not choose to make changes based on the findings of this study. I I may choose not to participate in this study or withdraw from it at any time without penalty. Signature Date If you have any questions or concerns, please contact Norma Baptista at (517) 355-9674 or (517) 663-2320 55 APPENDIX C 56 CRI-ADULT ANSWER SHEET Form: Actual ldeal Name Date / / Sex Age Marital Status Ethnic Group Education Part 1 Describe the problem or situation DN = Definitely No MN = Mainly No MY = Mainly Yes DY = Definitely Yes 1. Have you ever faced a problem like this before? [DN MN MY DY I 2. Did you know this problem was going to occur? l DN MN MY DY j 3. Did you have enough time to get ready to handle this problem? lDN MN MY DY ] 4. When this problem occurred. did you think of it as a threat? l DN MN MY DY I 5. When this problem occurred. did you think of it as a challenge? l DN MN MY DW 6. Was this problem caused by something you did? LDN MN MY DH 7. Was this problem caused by something someone else did? [RN MN MY DH 8. Did anything good come out of dealing with this problem? j DN MN MY DY J 9. Has this problem or situation been resolved? I DN MN MY Dfl l0. If the problem has been worked out. did it turn out all right for you? [ DN MN MY DY I Part 2 N = No. Not at all 0 = Yes. Once or twice 8 = Yes. Sometimes F = Yes. Fairly often 1 2 3 4 s 6 F“, a NOSFNOSFNOSFNOSFNOSFNOSFNOSFNOSF 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 NOSFNOSFNOSFNOSFNOSFNOSFNOSFNOSF 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 NOSFNOSFNOSFNOSFNOSFNOSFNOSFNOSF 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 NOSFNOSFNOSFNOSFNOSFNOSFNOSFNOSF 33 34 35 36 37 36 39 40 NOSFNOSFNOSFNOSFNOSFNOSFNOSFNOSF 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 NOSFNOSFNOSFNOSFNOSFNOSFNOSFNOSF MB Psychological Assessment Resources, Ind/PD. Box 998/0dessa. FL 33556/ron-rree 1.300.331-rtsr Copyright '4': I993 by Psychological Assessment Resources. Inc. All rights reserved. May not be reproduced in whole or in part in any form or by any means without written permission of Psychological Assessment Resources. Inc. 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 Printed in the USA This form is printed in blue ink on carbonless paper. Any other version is unauthorized. Reorder fl RO—2329 Toll Free 180033l-TEST 57 Part 1 This booklet contains questions about how you manage important problems that come up in your life. Please think about the most important problem or stressful situation you have experienced in the last 12 months (for example, troubles with relative or fiiend, the illness of a relative or fi'iend, an accident or illness, financial or work problems). Briefly describe the problem in the space provided in Part 1 of the answer sheet. If you have not experienced a major problem, list minor problem that you have had to deal with. Then answer each of the 10 questions about the problem or situation (listed below and again on the answer sheet) by circling the appropriate response: 8. 9. Circle "DN" if your response is DEFINITELY NO. DN MN MY DY Circle MN ifyour response is MAINLY NO. 9“ MN MY DY Circle "MY" if your response is MAINLY YES. DN MN (My DY Circle "DY" if your response is DEFINITELY NO. DN MN MY GYD . Have you ever faced a problem like this before? Did you know this problem was going to occur? Did you have enough time to get ready to handle this problem? When this problem occurred, did you think of it as a threat? When this problem occurred, did you think of it as a challenge? Was this problem caused by something you did? Was this problem caused by something someone else did? Did anything good come out of dealing with this problem? Has this problem or situation been resolved? 10. If the problem has been worked out, did it turn out all right for you? 58 Part 2 Read each item carefully and indicate how often you engaged in that behavior in connection with the problem you described in Part 1. Circle the appropriate response on the answer sheet: Circle "N" if your response is NO. Not at all. N) O S F Circle "0" if your response is YES. Once or Twice. N G) S F . . . . N 0 (S) F Circle "S" if your response IS YES, Sometimes. N o . Q) Circle "F" if your response is YES, Fairly often. There are 48 items Part 2. Remember to mark all your answers on the answer sheet. Please answer each item as accurately as you can. All your answers are strictly confidential. If you not wish to answer an item, please circle the number of that item on the answer sheet to indicate that you have decided to skip it. If an item does not apply to you, please write NA (Not Applicable) in the box to the right of the number for that item. If you wish to change an answer, make an X through your original answer and circle the new answer. Note that answers are numbered across in rows on Part 2 on the answer sheet. 1. Did you think of different ways to deal with the problem? 2. Did you tell yourself things to make yourself feel better? 3. Did you talk to your spouse or other relative about the problem? 4. Did you make a plan of action and follow it? 5. Did you try to forget the whole thing? 6. Did you feel that time would make a difference-that the only thing to do was to wait? 7. Did you try to help others deal with a similar problem? 8. Did you take it out on other people when you felt angry or depressed? 9. Did you try to step back from the situation and be more objective? 10. Did you remind yourself how much worse things could be? 59 ll. 12. 13. 14. 15. 16. 17. 18. 19. 20. 21. 22. 23. 24. 25. 26. 27. 28. 29. 30. 31. 32. 33. 34. 35. Did you talk with a friend about the problem? Did you know what had to be done and try to make things work? Did you try not to think about the problem/ Did you realize that you had no control over the problem? Did you get involved in new activities? Did you take a chance and do something risky? Did you go over in your mind what you would say or do? Did you try to see the good side of the situation? Did you talk with a professional person (e. g., doctor, lawyer, clergy)? Did you decide what you wanted and try hard to get it? Did you daydream or imagine a better time or place than the one you were in? Did you think that the outcome would be decided by fate? Did you try to make new fiiends/ Did you keep away from people in general? Did you try to anticipate how things would turn out? Did you think about how you were much better off than other people with similar problem? Did you seek help from persons or groups with the same type of problem? Did you try at least two different ways to solve the problem] Did you try to put off thinking about the situation, even though you knew you would have to at some point? Did you accept it; nothing could be done? Did you read more often as a source of enjoyment? Did you yell or shout to let off steam? Did you try to find some personal meaning in the situation? Did you try to tell yourself that things would get better? Did you try to find out more about the situation? 60 36. 37. 38. 39. 40. 41. 42. 43. 45. 46. 47. 48. Did you try to learn more things on your own? Did you wish that the problem would go away or somehow be over with? Did you expect the worst possible outcome? Did you expend more time in recreational activities? Did you cry to let your feelings out? Did you try to anticipate the new demands that would be placed on you? Did you think about how this event could change your life in a positive way? Did you pray for guidance and/or strength? . Did you take things a day at a time, one step at a time? Did you try to deny how serious the problem really was? Did you lose hope that things would ever be the same? Did you turn to work or other activities to help you manage things? Did you do something that you didn't think would work, but at least you were doing something? 61 APPENDIX D 62 CRl-ADULT PROFILE TSCORE RAW RAWZ RAW3 RAW4 RAWS RAWG RAW?— RAWB TSCORE ‘ 80+ 14-18 80 79 13 79 78 18 78 77 77 76 18 17 76 75 18 12 75 74 17 16 74 73 17 73 72 18 16 11 72 71 15 71 70 16 70 69 17 15 14 10 69 68 18 68 67 18 16 18 14 15 13 67 66 9 66 65 17 17 13 14 65 64 15 17 12 64 63 13 8 63 62 16 16 14 16 12 11 62 61 12 61 60 15 15 15 11 10 7 60 59 13 11 59 58 14 10 9 58 57 14 12 14 6 57 56 13 10 56 55 13 13 9 8 SS 54 11 9 5 S4 53 12 8 7 53 52 52 10 12 8 52 51 11 7 6 4 51 50 11 11 50 49 10 9 7 49 48 10 6 5 3 48 47 10 9 8 6 47 46 5 4 46 4S 8 9 2 45 44 9 7 5 3 44 43 8 4 43 42 8 7 6 4 2 1 42 41 7 3 41 40 6 3 40 39 7 5 2 1 O 39 38 S 6 38 37 6 4 1 2 0 37 36 4 5 36 35 1 35 34 5 3 3 4 0 34 33 0 33 32 4 2 32 31 2 3 31 30 30 29 3 1 1 2 29 28 0 28 27 2 0 1 27 26 26 25 25 24 1 0 24 23 23 22 0 22 21 21 20 20 1A.__L..SF 1" LAB—.58 ED 63 APPENDIX E Table 7 LA PR S6 PS EA AR SR ED ' Pearson. LA 1.000 .426“ .555“ .545“ -.032 -.145 .233" .099 Correlation PR .426“ 1.000 .504*1 .469“ .064 -.203 .296* -.108 56 .555" .504*1 1.000 .513“ -.028 -.033 .020 .002 PS .545“ .469“ .513" 1.000 -.164 -.331** .097 .141 CA -.032 .064 -.028 -.164 1.000 .473" .157 284* AR -.145 -.203 -.033 -.331** .473" 1.000 -.223 278* SR .233* .296* .020 .097 .157 -.223 1.000 .160 ED .099 --108 .002 2141 .284* 178* .160 1.000 Sig. (Z-tailed) LA . .000 .000 .000 .789 .221 .049 .405 PR .000 . .000 .000 .591 .085 .012 .364 so .000 .000 . .000 .816 .781 .870 .989 PS .000 .000 .000 . .169 .005 .421 .238 CA .789 .591 .816 .169 . .000 .189 .015 AR .221 .085 .781 .005 .000 . .060 .017 SR .049 .012 .870 .421 .189 .060 . .179 ED 305 364 -989 .238 .015 .017 -179 . N LA 73 73 73 72 73 73 72 73 PR 73 73 73 72 73 73 72 73 so 73 73 73 72 73 73 72 73 PS 72 72 72 72 72 72 71 72 CA 73 73 73 72 73 73 72 73 AR 73 73 73 72 73 73 72 73 SR 72 72 72 71 72 72 72 72 ED 72 72 72 7? 72 72 72 72 **,Corre|ation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed). *, Correlation is significant at the 0.05 level (Z-tailed). 65 Table 8 Levene's Test for Egualigy of Variances t-test for E uality of Means 95% Confidence Interval of the Mean Mean Std. Error Sig. df Si . (Z-tagiled) Difference Difference LOWE! Upper LA Equal variances assumed Equal variances not .___mms_d 1.886 .174 -.558 —.629 71 1 5.462 .579 .538 -1.5205 -1 .5205 2.7250 2.41 58 -6.9540 -6.6562 3.91 30 3.6152 PR Equal van ances assumed Equal variances not .____assem .061 .806 -2.089 -2.316 71 15.183 .040 .035 -5.8563 ~5.8563 2.8040 2.5286 1 1.4473 1 1.2403 -.2653 -.4 723 56 Equal variances assumed Equal vanances not .437 .511 -.912 -.928 71 13.997 .365 .369 -3.3299 -3.3299 3.6530 3.5894 10.6139 1 1.0287 3.9540 4.3689 . ggumed PS Equal variances assumed Equal variances not . ____assymssl .040 .841 -.741 —.677 70 12.940 .461 .510 -2.0596 -2.0596 2.7796 3.0433 -7.6034 -8.6374 3.4842 4.5182 CA Equal vaflances assumed Equal variances not ‘ 8551111189 3.884 .053 -.948 -1.294 71 20.157 .346 .210 -2.7185 -2.7185 2.8677 2.1007 -8.4364 -7.0982 2.9995 1.6613 AR Equal variances assumed Equal variances not 1.615 .208 .527 .607 71 1 5.831 .553 1 .4648 1 .4648 2.7813 2.4144 -4.0810 -3.6580 7.0107 6.5876 SR Equal variances assumed Equal variances not . gsgmgg .039 .845 -.770 -.716 70 1 3.099 .444 .487 -2.2101 -2.2101 2.8700 3.0874 -7.9342 -8.8751 w 3.5139 4.4548 ED Equal variances assumed Equal variances not __assumed .964 .329 .236 .207 71 12.558 .814 .839 .7933 .7933 3.3542 3.8281 -5.8948 -7.5066 7.481 3 9.0931 *CA Shows statistically significant difference (F +3. 889,P> .,053) with students reporting academic difficulties using more cognitive avoidance than students who did n0t report academic difficulties. No statistically significant difference between Approach coping responses and the students reporting academic difficulties. 66 Table 9 Levene‘s Test for F Egualig of Variances 5&- df LA Equal variances assumed Equal variances not . _mumed .196 .659 1.311 1.311 71 70.41 0 233' .194 .194 t-testfor ual ofMeans ##— Std. Error Difference Mean Difference 2.5338 2.5338 1 .9324 1 .9330 9i Confidence Lower -1.3192 -1.3210 mm of the M Jam. 6.3868 6.3887 PR Equal variances assumed Equal variances not .457 .501 1 .080 1 .082 71 70.952 .284 .283 2.2158 2.2158 2.0519 2.04 70 -1.8755 -1.8658 6.3071 6.2974 50 Equal variances assumed Equal variances not . .___mum 1.932 2.438 2.400 71 58.513 .017 .020 6.1632 6.1 632 2.5274 2.5683 1.1236 1.0231 1 1.2027 1 1.3033 PS Equal variances assumed Equal variances not . __mumssl .085 .771 1 .403 70 69.090 .165 .165 2.7833 2.7833 1.9833 1 .9835 -1.1723 -1.1736 6.7389 6.7402 CA Equal variances assumed Equal variances not 8.691 2.827 2.861 71 68.1 58 5.5391 5.5391 1.9591 1 .9360 1 .6328 1 .6760 9.4454 9.4022 AR Equal variances assumed Equal variances not , ____mmssl .496 .484 1.122 1.126 71 70.985 .265 .264 2.2203 2.2203 1 .9781 1 .9724 -1.7239 -1.7127 6.1645 6.1533 SR Equal variances assumed Equal variances not .065 .800 -.410 -.410 70 69.939 .683 .683 -.8486 -.8486 2.0722 2.0706 -4.9814 —4.9784 3.2841 3.281 1 ED Equal variances assumed Equal variances not .635 .428 2.137 2.146 71 70.966 .036 .035 4.9767 4.9767 2.3291 2.3189 .3326 .3528 9.6208 9.6006 . __assumed *CA shows statistically significant difference (F+8.691,P >004). No statiStically significant difference between Approach coping responses and the students' gender. 67 Table 10 Levene's Test for Egualig of Variances an df [PR SC [AR SR ‘ED .____ass.umssl .___mmssl __assumad Equal variances assumed Equal variances not 3.691 .059 .489 .526 71 55.408 Z-Stfil'ed .626 .601 t-test for Equality 0 Mean Difference 1.0162 1.0162 fMeans ,__L Std. Error Difference _ 2.0762 1.9312 95% Confidence Lower -3.1237 —2.8534 Interval of the U53 5.1560 4.8857 Equal variances assumed Equal variances not .211 .648 -.649 -.626 71 41.869 .519 .535 -1.4226 -1.4226 2.1934 2.2718 -5.7962 -6.0077 2.9510 3.1625 Equal variances assumed Equal variances not m .016 .900 -.235 —.245 71 51.164 .815 .807 -.6582 -.6582 2.7970 2.6816 -6.2353 -6.041 3 4.9190 4.7249 Equal variances assumed Equal variances not .329 .568 -.861 -.864 70 43.545 .392 .392 -1.8438 -1.8438 2.1419 2.1339 -6.1157 -6.1458 2.4280 2.4581 Equal variances assumed Equal variances not m 3.762 .056 .832 .926 71 60.440 .408 .358 1.8197 1.8197 2.1870 1 .9650 -2.5409 -2.1102 6.1804 5.7497 Equal vanances assumed Equal vaflances not .032 .859 1.417 1 .444 71 48.166 .161 .155 2.9651 2.9651 2.0928 2.0527 -1.2079 -1.1618 7.1381 7.092 1 Equal vafiances assumed Equal vafiances not 1.063 .306 .066 .061 70 37.557 .947 .952 .1458 .1458 2.1996 2.3855 -4.241 1 -4.6852 4.5327 4.9768 Equal variances assumed Equal variances not .086 .770 .469 .452 71 41.678 .654 1.1956 1.1956 2.5514 2.6474 -3.8917 -4.1482 6.2829 6.5394 No statiStically significant difference between Approach coping responses and students' age. 68 Table 11 LeveneTs Test for uaii of Variances t-testfor ual ofM s _ 95% Confidence 51. Mean Std. Error M3! the F 519. t df 2431 Difference Difference Lower U r LA Equal variances .018 .893 -1.670 71 .099 —3.2081 1.9212 -7.0389 .6226 assumed Equal variances -1.655 66.332 .103 -3.2081 1.9383 —7.0777 .6614 ‘ nm assume; PR Equal variances .199 .657 .463 71 .645 .9570 2.0687 -3.1678 5.0818 assumed Equal variances .465 70.568 .644 .9570 2.0599 —3.1508 5.0648 '56 Equal variances .619 .434 -.052 71 .959 -.1365 2.6351 -5.3907 5.1177 assumed Equal variances -.052 70.842 .959 -.1365 2.6184 -5.3576 5.0846 ‘PS Equal variances 1.823 .181 1.052 70 .297 2.1026 1.9992 -1.8847 6.0898 assumed Equal variances 1.065 69.961 .291 2.1026 1.9749 -1.8363 6.0414 ‘CA Equal variances 9.138 .003 -i.510 71 .136 -3.0754 2.0371 -7.1373 .9865 assumed Equal variances -1.547 67.625 .126 -3.0754 1.9876 -7.0419 .8911 . not assumed AR Equal variances .288 .593 -1.133 71 .261 -2.2436 1.9808 -6.1931 1.7059 assumed Equal variances -1.139 70.744 .259 -2.2436 1.9700 -6.1718 1.6846 ‘ sum SR Equal variances .276 .601 -.672 70 .504 -1.3916 2.0704 -5.5209 2.7376 assumed Equal variances -.668 66.401 .507 -1.3916 2.0845 -5.5531 2.7698 ‘ED Equal variances .042 .839 -1.498 71 .139 -3.5483 2.3693 -8.2726 1.1760 assumed . Equal variances -1.497 69.561 .139 —3.5483 2.3701 -8.2758 1.1792 No statistically significant difference between Approach coping responses and students' ages 21 to 25. *CA shows statistically significant difference (F +9.138,P>.003). 69 TablelZ Levene‘s Test for Equality of Variances 51. df LA Equal variances assu Equal variances not . .____.i§.illm£.il 3.691 .059 .489 .526 71 55.408 _ 5233i“; .626 .601 t-test for Quely of Means 1.0162 1.0162 Difference Std. Error Difference 2.0762 1.9312 Lower -3.1237 -2.8534 — 95—! Confidence Interval of the Mean Dear 5.1 560 4.8857 PR Equal variances assumed Equal variances not .211 .648 -.649 -.626 71 41.869 .519 .535 -1 .4226 -1 .4226 2.1934 2.2718 -5.7962 -6.0077 2.9510 3.1625 SC Equal variances assumed Equal variances not .016 -.235 -.245 71 51.164 .815 .807 -.6582 -.6582 2.7970 2.6816 -6.2353 -6.041 3 4.9190 4.7249 PS Equal vanances asuuned Equal vanances not .___auumssi .329 .568 -.864 70 43.545 .392 .392 -1 .8438 -1.8438 2.1419 2.1339 -6.1157 -6.1458 2.4280 2.4581 Equal variances assumed Equal variances not mm! 3.762 .056 .832 .926 71 60.440 .408 .358 1 .8197 1.8197 2.1870 1 .9650 -2.5409 -2.1 102 6.1804 5.7497 AR Equal variances assumed Equal variances not .032 .859 1.417 1 .444 71 48.166 .161 .155 2.9651 2.9651 2.0928 2.0527 -1 .2079 -1.1618 7.1381 7.0921 SR Equal variances assumed Equal variances not 1 .063 .306 .066 70 37.557 .947 .952 .1458 .1458 2.1996 2.3855 -4.241 1 -4.6852 4.5327 4.9768 ‘ ED Equal variances assumed Equal variances not .086 .770 .469 .452 71 41.678 .641 .654 1.1956 1.1956 2.5514 2.6474 -3.8917 -4.1482 6.2829 6.5394 No statistically significant difference between Approach coping responses and students' ages 26 to 30. *CA shows some statistically significant difference (F + 3.72,P> .56). 70 Table 13 Levene's Test for Egualig of Variances t-test for Equality o Sig: df Si . (2-ta?1ed) if Mea__ns Mean Difference Std. Error Difference 95% Confidence Lower interval of the Mean Upper LA Equal variances assumed Equal variances not assum 1.889 .174 1.557 .913 71 2.054 .124 .455 7.5333 7.5333 4.8396 8.2473 -2.1166 -27.0663 17.1833 42.1330 PR Equal variances assumed Equal variances not in 3.020 .087 1.510 4.555 71 5.212 .135 .005 7.7381 7.7381 5.1244 1 .6988 -2.4796 3.4240 17.9558 12.0522 SC Equal variances assumed Equal variances not . assumed 1.047 .310 .285 .196 71 2.077 .777 .862 1.8857 1 .8857 6.61 78 9.6301 -11.3098 -38.1090 15.0813 41.8804 PS Equal variances assumed Equal variances not as um .891 .349 .596 .823 70 2.366 .553 .485 2.9855 2.9855 5.0114 3.6294 -7.0095 -10.5318 12.9805 16.5028 CA Equal variances assumed Equal variances not assumed .000 .992 1.575 1.309 71 2.117 .120 .315 8.0524 8.0524 5.1119 6.1512 -2.1404 -17.0625 18.2452 33.1672 AR Equal vaflances assunned Equal vanances not ‘ assumgg 2.535 .116 -.758 -.451 71 2.056 .451 .695 -3.7905 -3.7905 5.0019 8.4112 -13.7640 -39.0519 6.1 830 31.4709 SR Equal vaflances assunned Equal vaflances not ._____as12ms1 1.653 .203 1.378 3.119 70 3.268 .172 .047 7.0580 7.0580 5.1201 2.2626 -3.1537 .1806 1 7.2696 13.9353 ED Equal variances assumed Equal variances not __asallmad 1.621 .207 1.259 2.323 71 2.721 .212 .112 7.5286 7.5286 5.9806 3.2406 —4.3964 -3.4099 19.4536 1 8.4671 *PR shows some statistically significant difference (P +3.020,P>.87). 71 Table 14 LevenersTest for Equality of Variances t-te_st ii lr Egualitv of Means S‘L df Si . (2-ta?led2 PR SC AR SR .____as§_llmd .._____a§.:.lim_c.d Equal variances assumed Equal variances not .722 .398 -2.592 -2.382 71 29.450 .012 .024 Mean Difference Std. Error Difference 95% Confidence JL_ower interval of the Mean Upper -5.4274 -5.4274 2.0937 2.2 782 -9.6021 -10.0838 -1.2526 -.7710 Equal variances assumed Equal variances not .____a§.§u_m_c1 .516 .475 -1.326 -1.387 71 37.571 .189 .174 -3.0349 -3.0349 2.2890 2.1884 -7.5991 -7.4667 1.5293 1.3969 Equal variances assumed Equal variances not as umed .606 .439 .020 .019 71 33.274 .984 .985 .7556-02 .755E-02 2.9473 2.9924 -5.8191 -6.0286 5.9342 6.1436 Equal vanances assumed Equal vaflances not u 1.348 .250 -.599 -.673 70 40. 540 .551 .505 -1.3605 -1.3605 2.2721 2.0223 -5.8920 -5.4459 3.1710 2.7250 Equal variances assumed Equal variances not assumed .025 .876 —1.421 -1.403 71 33.407 .170 -3.2434 -3.2434 2.2825 2.3125 -7.7945 -7.9461 1.3078 1 .4593 Equal variances assumed Equal variances not .993 .322 .217 .202 71 30.058 .829 .842 .4840 .4840 2.2346 2.4016 -3.9717 4.4204 4.9396 5.3883 Equal variances assumed Equal variances not 2.870 .095 -3.176 -3.774 70 51.414 .002 -6.8731 -6.8731 2.1644 1.8214 -11.1898 -10.5289 -2.5563 -3.2173 ‘ assumed ED Equal variances assumed Equal variances not 2.946 .090 -2.846 -3.254 71 46.109 .006 .002 -7.2575 -7.2575 2.5500 2.2306 -12.3420 -11.7472 -2.1730 -2.7679 No statistically significant difference between Approach and Avoidance coping responses and the students' marital status. 72 TablelS LeveneTs Test for Equality of Variances slq. Equal variances assumed Equal variances not .130 .719 -1.714 -1.743 69 48.637 gz-i'all'adz .091 .088 t-test for Qualig oif Means F__ Mean Difference -3.5408 -3.5408 Std. Error Difference 2.0661 2.0312 -7.6626 -7.62 34 95§Confidence inmal of the Lower Upar .581 1 .5419 PR Equal variances assumed Equal variances not .018 .895 -.323 -.320 69 45.177 .748 .751 -.7066 -.7066 2.1879 2.2102 -5.0713 -5.1577 3.6582 3.7446 SC Equal variances assumed Equal variances not 2.797 .099 -2.597 -2.238 32.199 .011 .032 -6.9353 -6.93 53 2.6704 3.0991 -12.2627 -1 3.2464 -1 .6079 -.6242 PS Equal variances assumed Equal variances not .095 .759 -2.725 -2.778 49.309 .008 .008 -5.5272 -5.5272 2.0282 1 .9897 -9.5743 -9.5249 -1.4800 -1.5295 Equal variances assumed Equal variances not .812 .371 1.771 1 .909 69 56.803 .081 .061 3.7757 3.7757 2.1323 1 .9783 -.4782 -.1860 8.0296 7.7374 AR Equal variances assumed Equal variances not .025 .876 1 .998 2.008 69 47.091 .050 .0 50 4.0691 4.0691 2.0370 2.0265 .414E-03 7.39E-03 8.1329 8.1457 SR Equal variances assumed Equal variances not . __aaslimesl .369 .545 .713 .674 68 40.005 .478 .504 1.5471 1.5471 2.1685 2.2970 -2.7800 -3.0954 5.8742 6.1896 ED Equal variances assumed Equal variances not __assllmeL 1.963 .166 1.546 1 .470 69 40.677 .127 .149 3.7979 3.7979 2.4570 2.5828 -1.1037 -1.4195 8.6995 9.0153 No statistically significant difference bctween Approach and Avoidance coping responses and the students' race. 73 Table 16 Levene's Test for Equality of Variances t-test for Equality of Means 95% Confidence Mean Std. Error interval of the Mean Si . F Sig; t df (2-taglled) Difference Difference Lower Upper LA Equal variances .724 .398 .191 70 .849 .3923 2.0540 -3.7042 4.4889 assumed Equal 3153““ .184 44.433 .855 .3923 2.1282 -3.8956 4.6803 . assumgq PR Equal variances .179 .674 -.669 70 .506 -1.4672 2.1933 -5.8416 2.9071 assumed Equal :zrtiances -.690 53.514 .493 -1.4672 2.1268 -5.7320 2.7975 umed ‘SC Equal variances 2.978 .089 .199 70 .843 .5549 2.7930 -S.0156 6.1254 assumed Equal mm” .174 34.749 .863 .5549 3.1828 -5.9082 7.0180 assumgg PS Equal variances 1.679 .199 -1.319 69 .191 -2.7470 2.0824 -6.9013 1.4074 assumed Equal mums -1.412 59.598 .163 -2.7470 1.9454 -6.6388 1.1449 sum ‘CA Equal variances .075 .785 -.905 70 .369 -1.9745 2.1829 -6.3282 2.3792 assumed Equal mm“ -.928 52.799 .357 -1.9745 2.1269 -6.2410 2.2920 assumed ‘ AR Equal variances .382 .538 -.423 70 .674 -.8860 2.0940 -5.0623 3.2904 assumed Equal mm” -.416 46.901 .679 -.8860 2.1280 -5.1672 3.3953 ‘ §§§ngg SR Equal variances 1.246 .268 1.503 69 .138 3.2548 2.1662 -1.0667 7.5762 assumed Equal mums 1.608 59.598 .113 3.2548 2.0236 -.7937 7.3032 . §S§Qfll§d ED Equal variances 1.232 .271 .555 70 .580 1.3617 2.4514 -3.5275 6.2509 assumed Equal mum” .588 57.438 .559 1.3617 2.3161 -3.2755 5.9989 _asaumaii No statistically significant difference between Approach and Avoidance coping responses and students' year in school. 74 BIBLIOGRAPHY Bandura, A. (1977). Self-Efficacy: Towards a Unifying Theory of Behavioral Change, Psychological Review, 51, 1173-82. Billings, A., and Moos, R., Life Stressors and Social Resources Affect Postreatment Outcomes Among Depressed Patients. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 94, 1985. Brammer, Lawrence M., How to Cope with Life Transitions, The Challenge of Personal Change, Hemisphere Publishing Corporation, 1990. Brown, C. W., and Harris, T., Social Origins of Depression, London: Tavislock, 1978. Coombs, Robert H. and St. John, Joanne, Making it in Medical School, Spectrum Publications, INC., 1979. Cooper, Cary L. and Payne, Roy, Personality and Stress: Individual Differences in the Stress Process, John Wiley, 1991. Cuca, J. M., Sakakeeny, L. A., and Johnson, D. G., The Medical School Admissions Process: A Review of the Literature, 1955-1976. Washington D. C.: Association of American Medical Colleges. Dohrenwend, B. S., and Dohrenwend, B. P., Division 27 Award for Distinguished Contributions to Community Psychology and Community Mental Health, American Journal Community Psychiatry, 9(1981): 123-164. Eggert, L. L., Support in Family Ties: Stress, Coping, and Adaptation. In Albrecht and Adelman (Eds), Communicating Social Support, Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Elam, Carol L., Medical Students and Stress, The Advisor, Summer 1994/Vol. 14. 75 Endler, Norman 5., and Parker, James D. A., Assessment of Multidimensional Coping: Task, Emotion, and Avoidance Strategies. Psychological Assessment, 1994. Fondacaro, M., and Moos, R., Social Support and Coping: A Longitudinal Analysis. American Journal of Community Psychology, 15, 1987. Gaensbauer, T. J. and Mizner, G. L., "Developmental Stress in Medical Education," Psychiatry 43, 1980. Gottlieb, B., Social Networks and Social Support, Beberly Hills, CA: Sage, 1981. Gottlieb, B., Social Support and Risk ReduCtion. Journal of Primary Prevention, 3, 71- 76, 1982. Hirsch, B. J., Natural Support Systems and Coping with Major Life Changes. American Journal of Community Psychology, 8(1980). Holahan, C., and Moos, R., The Personal and Contextual Determinants of Coping Strategies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 1987. Holmes, T., and Rahe, R., The Social Adjustment Rating Scale. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 11, 213-218, 1967. Kahn, R. L. (1974). Conflict, Ambiguity and Over Work: Three Elements in Job Stress, In A. McLean (ed) Occupational Stress, Charles, C. Thomas, Sprigfield, Illinois. Kobassa, S., and Puccetti, M., Personality and Social Resources in Stress Resistance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 839-850, 1983. Lazarus, R., and Folkman, 8., Stress Appraisal and Coping, New York: Springer, 1984- 85. Mitchell, K. J., Use of MCAT Data in Selecting Students for Admissions to Medical School. Journal of Medical Education, 62(1987): 871-879. 76 Moos, Rudolf H., Coping Responses Inventory, Professional Manual, Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc., 1993. Mosley, T. H. Jr.; Perrin, S. G.; Neral, S. M.; Dubbert, P. M.; Grothues, C. A.; Pinto, B. M., Stress, Coping and Well-Being Among Third Year Medical Students. Academic Medicine, 1994 Sep; 69(9): 765-7. Pilisuk, Mark and Parks Hillier Susan, The Healing Web: Social Networks and Human Survival, University Press of New England, 1986. Rhode, P., Lewinshon, P., Tilson, M. and Seeley, J. (1990). Dimensionality of Coping and its Relation to Depression, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 499- 51 1. Rospenda, Kathleen M., Halpert, Jane and Richman, Judith A., Effects of Social Support on Medical Students' Performance, Academic Medicine, 69, 496-500, 1994. Sarason, B. R.; Pierce, G. R.; and Sarason, I. G., Social Support: The Sense of Acceptance and the Role of Relationships. In Sarason, Sarason and Pierce (Eds), Social Support: An Interactional View, New York: John Wiley, 1983. Selye, H., The stress of Life, New York: McGraw—Hill, 1956. Selye, H., Stress in Health and Disease. Boston Butterworths Publishers INC, 1976. Schneider, John, Stress, Loss 8: Grief, Maryland: University Park Press, 1984. Stewart, S.M., Betson, C., Marshall, I., Wong, C. M., Lee, P. W., and Lam, T. H., Stress and Vulnerability in Medical Students, Medical Education, 29(22):119-27, 1995. Suls, J. and Fletcher, B., The Relative Efficacy of Avoidant and non Avoidant Coping Strategies: A Meta-Analysis, Health Psychology, 4, 249-88, 1985. 77 Toews, John A., Lockyer, Jocelyn M., Dobson, Deborah J. G., Simpson, Elizabeth, Brownell A., Brenneis, Fraser, MacPherson, Kathleen M., and Cohen, Gerald 8., Analysis of Stress Levels among Medical Students, Residents, and Graduate Students at Four Canadian Schools of Medicine, Academic Medicine, 72, 997-1002, 1997. Weston, J. A. and Paterson, C. A., ”A Medical Student Support System at The University of Colorado School of Medicine,” Journal of Medical Education. 55, 1980. 78 "Illlllllllll'llllllllf