.... / .v... 1.. 1 INITIIIIIII\\\\\\\\;§\\\}§\T\\§T\\\\i r 3 1293 0078 LIBRARY Michigan State ‘_ University This is to certify that the dissertation entitled THE EFFECT OF PRIOR KNOWLEDGE AND LANGUAGE PROFICIENCY ON THE NONNATIVE SPEAKERS' WRITTEN DISCOURSE presented by AH SaIeh Khabti AI-Ghamdi has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for Ph.D. degreein EnQITSh - gm Major professor Date May 5, 1989 MSUl'rnnAffirm/n' A - r, m" 042771 Ad —_v4_7...4._,_.~*4-.‘A , PLACE IN RETURN BOX to remove this checkout from your record. TO AVOID FINES return on or before date due. DATE DUE DATE DUE DATE DUE m I! moi MSU Is An Affirmative Action/Equal Opportunity Institution c:\circ\datedm.pm3—p.1 ‘ THE EFFECT OF PRIOR KNOWLEDGE AND LANGUAGE PROFICIENCY ON THE NONNATIVE SPEAKERS' WRITTEN DISCOURSE By Ali Saleh Khabti Al—Ghamdi A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of English 1989 f§Q,rb:::uD ABSTRACT THE EFFECT OF PRIOR KNOWLEDGE AND LANGUAGE PROFICIENCY ON THE NONNATIVE SPEAKERS' WRITTEN DISCOURSE BY Ali Saleh Khabti Al—Ghamdi This study was designed to further investigate the clahms of Langer (1984), Chesky and Hiebert (1987), Stroethoff (1988), and others that. prior‘ knowledge is related to success in writing. Using 49 nonnative speakers studying English at the advanced levels in an intensive ESL program at a large, Midwestern university, the study investigated the relationships among various measurements of" writing' skill (writing jproficiency scores), background knowledge, language proficiency, reading habits, and demographic information. Each subject was asked to write on the topic of eclipses, a topic on which students showed a large spread 0f knowledge. Before writing the essay, the subjects took free-association and prior knowledge tests on the toPic. Then they filled out a questionnaire which reflected the degree of their involvement with the topic, their background characteristics, and the amount of Ali Saleh Khabti Al-Ghamdi reading they did. Trained raters assigned holistic writing proficiency scores and rated the writing samples for amount of content, text sophistication, global coherence, linguistic complexity, and revision strategies. The raters also scored the prior knowledge test for the degree of fluency and organization. Data were analyzed using frequency analysis, correlation coefficient, regression, and analysis of variance. IPrior knowledge significantly correlated with writing proficiency scores, content length, text sophistication, global coherence, and involvement. Prior knowledge was not significantly related with T-unit length, amount of subordination and revision strategies. Overall language proficiency was significantly correlated with writing proficiency scores, text sophistication, and linguistic complexity, but was not significantly related with content length, global coherence, revision strategies, and involvement. Among the other variables measured, age appears to have a significant relationship with content length and involvement with the text. Level of education and the amount of reading appear to have a significant relationship only with involvement. The results also showed that there was a difference among the subjects' Ali Saleh Khabti Al—Ghamdi writing related 1x3 their native languages. Implication of these findings are drawn and suggestions for further research are provided. Copyright by Ail s. K. Al—Ghamdi 1989 vi Dedicated to my father, Saleh, and my mother, Khadra. May God rest their souls in Heaven. My brother, Mohammad, and my wife, Azizah, with my love and thanks. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS At the successful completion of this project, I would like first to thank God for the opportunity, patience, and strength to enable me to finish this simple work. Thanks also goes to my government for its sponsorship and support, which made this work possible. I also wish to extend my sincere thanks and appreciation to all those who have helped me along the way. In particular, I am indebted to the following people: Dr. Paul.‘E. Munsell, friend, academic advisor, and committee chairman, for encouragement valuable comments, and helpful suggestions. Dr. James C. Stalker, Dr. Marcellette G. Villiams, and Dr. Marilyn Wilson for their willingness to :ake the time and responsibility to serve on my guidance :ommittee and for their thoughtful comments on my ussertation work. Dr. E. Dean Detrich for serving as a acuity representative for the College of Arts and etters at my dissertation's defense and for the comments e provided. Dr. Betsy Becker, for answering my statistical lestions during the analysis phase of this manuscript. viii Mrs. Ila Baker, Mrs. Alice Dungey, and Mr. Joel Boyd, Jr., for their time and effort to serve as raters for the study. Also my appreciation goes to Margot Hynes for her assistance as a rater. Director of the English Language Center, his staff, and D and E levels writing teachers and students of Spring, 1988, for their cooperation and help in collecting the data for this study. Nancy Heath for her cooperation and meeting of deadlines in the typing of this manuscript. Last, but not least, special thanks go to all my friends and relatives for their help, encouragement, and financial support. My appreciation goes to my wife, my aughters Abeer, Najla, and Ro'aa and my sons Saleh and ohammed for their love, support, and patience. ix Table of Contents Page :IISTOFTABLES..eenooeooeooeoo'boao-oeeoooosq ...... XiV LIST OFFIGURES................... ........ . ....... xvii LIST OF APPENDICES..................... ..... .....Xviii Ihapter I. INTRODUCTION...................... ..... .......l The Problem.................................1 Reading and Writing.........................2 The Goal of the Study.................. ..... 5 Hypotheses..................................6 Limitation of the Study... ..... . ..... .......7 Generalizability of the Study...............7 SumarYOooeoeeouoooeooo-ocooooooooo ....... .08 II. REVIEWOF LITERATURE.CIOOIOCOOOOIODOOOC.00... 9 Introduction ..... ...... ..... ... ........ .... 9 Prior Knowledge. .......... ......... ..... 9 Prior Knowledge and Reading. . ... ....... 9 Top— —Down Processing in Reading. ..........10 Reader and Text Interaction.... .......... 11 In Explaining Schema Theory. ..... ........12 Schema Theory and L1 Reading........ ....... 14 Schema Theory and L2 Reading. .. ........ 16 Content Schema... ..... ...... ...... ....16 Content Schema vs. Formal Schema ........ .18 Cultural Schema........ ........... ..19 Schema Theory and Old Views of Reading. .21 Prior Knowledge and Writing Among Native Speakers............ ..............22 Writing and Topic Choice......... ........ 28 Language Proficiency and Writing Among Nonnative Speakers. ..... ...... ......31 Measuring Prior Knowledge and Writing Performance................... ...... .....34 Prior Knowledge.... ....... .... ...... .....34 Chapter Writing Measurement............. ........... T—Unit Length................ ..... ....... Amount of Subordination.................. Revision strategies....... ......... . ..... Global Coherence........ , Involvement.............................. Summary.................. III: METHODOLOGY.oe-tooctet-toe...ooeooo...ooooo-e The Study's Variables...................... Subjects. ................................ Materials .... ..... . ............... Selecting the Prompts for the Prior Knowledge Test. ........... Prior Knoweldge Test and Background Questionnaire........... ...... ...... Scoring the Subjects' Associations in the Prior Knowledge Test............ Writing Performance and Involvement Questionnaire.............. Writing Measurement................. ..... T—Unit Length and Amount of Subordination.......................... Writing Proficiency Scores. .............. Revision Strategies...................... Content..... ....... ..... ..... . ........ ... Global Coherence............. ........... . Text Sophistication.............. ..... ... Involvement. ..... ............ ..... Language Proficiency and Writing. ..... Data Analysis....................... ....... Summary...... ...... ........................ IV. RESULTS.............. ..... ... ..... .. ..... .... Hypotheses........ ........ ...... ....... Characteristics of the Participants. ...... . Characteristics of the Sample Performance. The Sample's Prior Knowledge Task........ The Characteristics of the Sample's Essays.......... ....... Relationships of Demographic Information and Amount of Reading with Writing... Comparisons by Native Language... ..... ..... Writing Proficiency Scores........ ......... Writing Proficiency Scores and Prior Knowledge.......... ..... ......... xi Chapter VI Writing Proficiency Scores and Overall Language Proficiency ........... 76 The Additive Effect ..................... 77 Content: Number of Words and Number of Ideas... . ............................... 79 Content and Prior Knowledge .............. 79 Number of Words .......................... 79 The Number of Ideas ...................... 81 Content and Overall Language Proficiency...83 Number of Words .......................... 83 The NUmber of Ideas ................... ...84 The Additive Effect ........ . ............. 84 Text Sophistication ........................ 86 Text Sophistication and Overall Language Proficiency ................... 89 The Additive Effect ...................... 90 Global Coherence ........................... 91 Global Coherence and Prior Knowledge.....91 Global Coherence and Overall Language Proficiency ................... 92 The Additive Effect ..................... 94 Linguistic Complexity: T- Unit Length and Subordination ........................ 96 Linguistic Complexity and Prior Knowledge .............................. 96 Linguistic Complexity and Overall Language Proficiency ................... 98 The Additive Effect ............... . ..... 99 Revision Strategies ...................... 101 Revision Strategies and Prior Knowledge ............................. 101 Revision Strategies and Overall Language Proficiency .................... 103 The Additive Effect ..................... 104 Involvement ............................... 105 Involvement and Prior Knowledge ......... 105 Involvement and Overall Language Proficiency ........................... 107 The Additive Effect ................ .....107 Summary ................................ ...108 CONCLUSIONS ................................. 111 Introduction .............................. 111 Personal Characteristics and Writing ...... 113 Participants' Naive Language ............ 114 Prior Knowledge and Writing Performance...115 Chapter Suggestions for Further Research. Summary... APPENDICES REFERENCES Writing Tests................ Content Length.... eoooooooooee Text Sophistication.......... Global Coherence.. Involvement... to. 00.00.0000... coo-0000.... o o o o o t o eeeee o o o o o e 9 0 clove... Linguistic Complexitiy................ Revision Strategies.......... Prior Knowledge and Writing Performance of the Subgroups...... Overall Language Proficiency and Writing Performance........ Additive Effect..... Implication........ coo-oo- tote-o. ....DIOIOOOOOOOOI eoeeooovoul eaeoeooooooucooeoooeo 0.300.000.- xiii coon-one. ego-coco .0000...- ooooleaoun 115 116 .117 118 119 120 121 121 122 .123 .124 127 130 .132 191 LIST OF TABLES Page Gender, Age, Level of Education, and Native Language.............. ........ ............ ....... 61 ELC Scores of Participants.................. ..... 62 Characteristics of the Sample's Prior Knowledge Task 64 Characteristics of the Sample's Essays...........65 The Relationship of Age, Level of Education, and Amount of Reading with Written Discourse ......68 Measures............... ......... oeoeooeeceo MANOVA and ANOVA Results for the Difference Between the Population Means Writing Measures by Native Language...............................7O Means of Organization, Combination, and Subordination by Native Language.................72 Means of Writing Proficiency Scores, Number of Words, Number of Ideas, Text Sophistication, Global Coherence, T—Unit Length, Revision Strategies, and Involvement of Writing Components by Native Language.........73 Correlation Between Prior Knowledge and Writing Proficiency Scores............ .........74 Correlation Between Overall Language Proficiency (OLP) and Writing Proficiency Scores.................. ........ Regression Results of Writing Proficiency ScoreSoooyeo ..... OIUOODIOOODIIO IIIIIIIIIII 0.9.0.078 . ....... 76 coo-cues: Correlation Between Prior Knowledge and Content: Number of words and Number of Ideas for all Groups and Each Group..............80 xiv Tab: Table 13. 14. 15. 16. l7. l8. 19. 20. 24. 25. 26. 28. Page Correlation Between Overall Language Number of Proficiency (OLP) and Content: Words and NUmber of Ideas............. ..........82 Regression Results of the Content: Number of Words......... ............ . . .. ............. . ..85 Regression Results of the Content: Number of Ideas............................................86 Correlation Between Prior Knowledge and Text Sophistication for All Groups and Each Group.....87 Correlation Between Overall Language Proficiency and Text Sophistication... ...... .....89 ...91 Regression Results of Text Sophistication..... Correlation Between Prior Knowledge and Global Coherence for All Groups and Each Group..........93 Correlation Between Overall Language Proficiency and Global Coherence.......... ....... 94 Regression Results of Global Coherence .......... 95 Correlation Between Prior Knowledge and T—unit Length and Linguistic Complexity: Subordination for All Groups and Each Group......97 Correlation Between Overall Language Proficiency (OLP) and Linguistic Complexity......98 Regression Results of the T—Unit Length ...... ..100 Regression Results of Subordination ......... ....101 Correlation Between Prior Knowledge and Revision Strategies for All Groups and Each Group... ................................... 102 Correlation Between Overall Language Proficiency (OLP) and Revision Strategies.......103 Regression Results of Revision Strategies.. ..... 105 XV Fable Page 29. Correlation Between Prior Knowledge and Involvement for All Groups and Each Group.......106 30. Correlation Between Overall Language Proficiency and Involvement.. ................... 107 31. Regression Results of Involvement... ............ 108 1-1. The Study's Raw Data ...... ..... ................. 178 LIST OF FIGURE Figure Page 1. Levels of organization and response categories: Schizophrenia......... .............36 xvii LIST OF APPENDICES Page Miscellaneous Correspondence.... ..... .........133 Background Information Questionnaire ........ ..140 Word Association/Prior Knowledge Test.. ....... 142 Scoring the Prompts............ ...... .........153 The Writing Task and the Involvement Questionnaire.......................... ....... 157 Scoring the Participants' Written Discourse Text Sophistication Questionnaire.... ..... ....161 Procedure for Selecting Prompts Procedures of Scoring Knowledge Organization. ... English Language Center Writing Scoring Scale Bamberg Coherence Scale. ............... . ..165 The Study's Raw Data....... ........ ...........175 ...179 Prior Knowledge Test and Writing Samples.. xviii Sp; CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION The Problem This study attempted to determine the relationship among prior knowledge, language proficiency, and writing of nonnative speakers of English. This investigation is important because no research has yet >een done with nonnative speakers. Most of the studies :ited below were conducted with native speakers. The .nvestigation is also important because the measurement if prior knowledge and its effect on writing have been eglected relative to the emphasis placed on language kills as summarized in the studies of Applebee (1984) nd Newell and MacAdam (1987). The more traditional view E writing tended to stress both language ability and rammar skills and equate them with writing ability. reffective writers were, therefore, often given a dose language in the hope that it would remedy their 'iting ailments. To achieve these language ends, one pic was often assigned to all students. As Chesky (1984) stated, however, when native eakers of English all wrote on the same topic, some 1 migh litt indi writ mech; Howei elems cruc: being discr and j varic When indie a giv View readii might not know* much about the topic, and hence, have little to write. Newell and MacAdam (1987) correctly indicated that the role of language proficiency in writing cannot be denied and that clarity of expression, and form are affected by language proficiency. also pointed out that these mechanics, However, these authors elements alone do not constitute writing ability. Other crucial factors, such as prior knowledge of the topic being written about, must be taken into account in discussing the writing ability of the students. Newell and MacAdam said, "Even good writers who have mastered various writing genres may seem to lack writing skills when facing new topics" (p. 157). Another writing theorist, Hoetker (1982) indicated that little has been known about the effect of a given topic on students' writing. He said: Our understanding of topic effects is at such a primitive stage that we cannot demonstrate whether or in what ways students will write differently in response to such thoughtless topics than they will write in response to the most carefully considered and closely edited topics (p. 382). Reading and Writing It seems more likely that this language-based iew of writing ignored a tremendous body of research on aading which assumed that reading involves much. more 1an language. As demonstrated in the reading research of (197 (198 cons the ever read rese stud: DeGr< (19M Speai becau compr Writt °Vers of' Johnson (1981), Rumelhart (1975), Anderson et a1. (1983), Lee (1986), Hudson (1982), Cziko (1977), Carrell (1980), and others, prior knowledge plays a major role in constructing meaning and organizing the information of the text. Based on the findings of these studies, there is every reason to believe that what has been found about reading probably applies to writing. Therefore, writing investigate writing . Some (1984), researchers have begun to studies of this type were conducted by Langer DeGroff (1986), Chesky and Hiebert (1987), and Stroethoff (1988) to clarify the role of prior knowledge in native speakers' writing. Langer (1984), for instance, effects on reading speculated that because there were schematic :omprehension, there were also schematic effects on written expression and ignoring such effects would be an >versight. She said: . . writing as a skill is too intertwined with knowledge of the subject matter itself to isolate the two without considering how one affects the other. Because topical knowledge so directly helps shape a paper, the teacher's understanding of what students know about a topic can be very useful :u1 planning writing assignments, in setting expectations for various students, and in providing pertinent help while a writer is at work (p. 28). Chesky and Hiebert (1987) examined the essays of :udents who wrote on two topics—-one in which the the: students had little knowledge and one in which they had considerable knowledge. Chesky and Hiebert found that when students wrote about a topic in which they had background knowledge, they wrote quantitatively more and qualitatively better; and they became more involved in their writing and enjoyed it more than when they wrote on a topic about which they had little knowledge. DeGroff (1986) investigated the effect of prior knowledge on conferencing' and revising texts and found that students with high knowledge produced better content by stating valued information within the specified (nowledge domain. Furthermore, the students with high :nowledge provided better comments during conferencing :essions. In comparing the effect of prior knowledge and :ohesive ties on the texts' coherence, Carrell (1982) ndicated that if students lacked suitable background nowledge, cohesive ties would not prevent their text rom being fragmented and incoherent. From these studies, we can conclude that writing asearchers have not gone far enough to look for the role 5 prior knowledge in nonnative speakers' writing. None these studies has looked at the effect of prior owledge and language proficiency on writing in spite of e opinion of Carrell (1986) and the other schema eorists that both prior knowledge (represented on top- wn processing) and language skills and vocabulary dev nee: knor non: spee The prof prof SOph revi: Profi inves rOle clear ident: effeci tentat effect Study relati info rm development (represented as bottom—up processing) are needed for comprehension to take place. Therefore, this study investigated whether prior knowledge represents the same type of importance in nonnative speakers' writing as it does in the native speakers' writing. Likewise, it also tests the effects of language proficiency on nonnative speakers' writing. The students' levels of prior knowledge and language proficiency will be correlated with their writing proficiency scores, length of content, text sophistication, global coherence, linguistic complexity, revision strategies, and involvement with the texts. The Goal of the Study Even though prior knowledge and language important and been their >roficiency would appear to be .nvestigated to some extent among native speakers, ole in the nonnative speakers' writing measures are not lear. The goal of this study then is to simultaneously dentify these roles. Without a study of the combined ffects of these two variables on writing measures, only antative predictions can be made about whether the ffects are separate or interactive. Furthermore, this :udy will undertake the investigation of the iationship among writing measures and demographic formation (such as age, gender, etc.), the amount of readi are i Engli reading students do and writing measures. The following are the major issues with regard to nonnative writers of English: The specific hypotheses Is there any difference among the participants' writing related to their personal characteristics and the amount of reading they do? What is the relationship between prior knowledge about specific topics and the nonnative speakers' written discourse? What is the relationship between overall language proficiency and the nonnative speakers' written discourse? What is the additive effect of prior knowledge and overall language proficiency on written discourse? Hypotheses (stated as null hypotheses) re the following: There is no relationship between the Hypothesis 1: speakers' prior knowledge about sophistication, linguistic complexity, and (g) involvement. nonnative specific topics and their scores in (a) writing proficiency tests, (b) content, (c) text (d) global coherence, (e) (f) revision strategies, 0rd get the Spa Hypothesis 2: There is no relationship between the nonnative speakers' overall language proficiency and (a) writing scores, (b) content, (c) text sophistication, (d) global coherence, (e) linguistic complexity, (f) revision strategies and involvement. Limitation of the Study A limitation is necessary related to applying the prior knowledge testing instrument to nonnative speakers. Swaffar (1988) said that students tested in their native language had ani advantage over their second language in recalling any'jpassage's information. Thus, because the subjects used their second language in the prior knowledge test, a decision of whether the students' responses to the test reflected their actual knowledge of eclipses or not can't be made. One potential solution to this limitation would have been to have the students use their first language in the prior knowledge test, then either translating the students' responses to English in arder to have them rated by English native raters or letting raters who speak the students' language to rate :heir responses. Generalizability of the Study The investigation was conducted with nonnative Peakers who were studying English as a second language t Michigan State University. There appears to be reason prt vie Vie alc wri eff var to believe that resulting generalizations can be applied to subjects of similar backgrounds. Summary This chapter stressed that emphasizing language proficiency and equating it with writing ability, as old Views of writing appear to have done, did not accurately represent the writing process. The recent views of writing, however, do not completely rule out the effect of language proficiency on writing. Instead, the recent views of writing indicate that language proficiency, along with students' prior knowledge, interact in the writing process. In this study we will examine the effects of both these variables on writing. Some other variables, such as the students' demographic information and the amount of reading the students do, are believed to be related ix: ESL written discourse; these variables will also be investigated. ant flO‘ lei C0] rei C011 int wri knc st 1an ul P app CHAPTER II REVIEW OF LITERATURE Introduction This study undertook the investigation of the effects of prior knowledge and language proficiency on nonnative speakers' writing. Concerning prior knowledge and its effects on writing, writing theorists generally now assume similarity between reading and writing at some level of abstraction since both. processes are used to construct meaning. Thus, the first section of this review of the literature will focus on the studies related to prior knowledge and its effects on reading comprehension. The second section of this review will include a recapitulation of the studies related to writing as a process affected by the writer's prior cnowledge. The third section of this chapter reviews the :tudies related to the measurement of grammar and .anguage proficiency on writing. Prior Knowledge rior Knowledge and Reading In outdated views of reading, readers passively pproached the texts in order to absorb what the authors sen‘ part thai resa Semi pro< com! rec: reat 393: the: reac‘ 0rde util "to 10 of those texts had produced. Those views were challenged by recent language and reading theorists who indicated that reading comprehension required an active participation. of the comprehenders. This argument was initiated by Chomsky (1959) who characterized the behaviorists' view of language as a type of learned human behavior with a predictable response to a known stimulus. Chomsky insisted that understanding and producing a sentence did not rely on our having learned that particular sentence. We can say and comprehend materials that we have not heard before. As a result of Chomsky's research, the emphasis of teaching language shifted from sentences themselves and their meaning to the learning processes of the students themselves, and how they comprehended sentences. This shift can be readily recognized in the consequent theories in the field of reading. Top—Down Processing in Reading Chomsky's research was followed by other reading theorists, such as Goodman (1967) who stated that reading LS a "psycholinguistic guessing game." Goodman said that readers must interact with the text they were reading in Irder to construct Ineaning. This interaction requires tilizing higher level knowledge, to enable the reader to get meaning from the print, to develop the sampling, pred para Good and grap was made the know] resee EVOIV Struc text delin aids (D 3’. (D thee; text With c°mpre two N( Calle< 11 predicting, confirming, and correcting strategies parallel to those they use in listening" (Goodman and Goodman, 1982, p. 75). So, according to Goodman (1967) and Goodman and Goodman (1982), it was not the text's graphic representation that caused comprehension, but it was the knowledge that readers brought to the text that made reading possible. A large amount of research has been conducted in the past decade to assess the effects of the readers' knowledge on reading comprehension. As a result of this research, a new, interesting and sophisticated theory evolved called Schema Theory. This theory demonstrated the necessity of an active utilization of knowledge structures within the comprehender in response to the text to be comprehended. The following section will delineate the way this interaction takes place and how it aids reading comprehension. Reader and Text Interaction Carrell (1986) said the way in which schema theory conceptualizes the interaction between reader and text is that the input of the text must be compatible with the reader's existing schema in order for comprehension to occur. Schema theory provides us with two modes by which information is processed. One mode is :alled bottom-up or data-driven processing which rep inf top rep rea comi faci know the thos proc know theo mode comp r6co Pr0c SChen infOI the 5 readE 12 represents the reader‘s utilization of the graphic information in the input data. The other mode is called top—down or conceptually-driven processing which represents the concepts from within the reader that the reader draws on to search the data. According to schema theory, top—down. processing is when readers make guesses or predictions as to what is coming in the text. Top—down processing is enhanced and facilitated by the reader's past experience or prior knowledge. Bottom-up processing is when readers decode the text's graphic components to construct meaning out of those linguistically decodable sentences. Bottom-up processing is .enhanced by the reader's linguistic knowledge (phonology, morphology, and syntax). Schema theory clearly suggests that relying on one processing mode at the expense of the other will impede comprehension (Eskey, 1988). However, readers must recognize that different texts may require more of one processing mode than the other. In Explaining Schema Theory Anderson (1984) enumerates six functions of schema. First, a schema is composed of slots. Learning Lnformation easily takes place if that information fits :he slots in the reader's schema. Second, schema enables eaders to pay more attention to what aspects of the text are mak hel inf rec cas aPP wou Fif infw Sixi Spet post SOme ment the What body exte COmp theo revi 13 are more important. Third, a schema enables readers to make inferences if the text is complex. Fourth, a schema helps direct readers to search their memory for certain information” As an example, we search our Inemory to recall what kind of food a fine meal includes. In this case and as a result of a inemory search, the kind of appetizer, the kind of soup, and whether or not a salad would be served are the types of information recalled. Fifth, a schema enables the reader to summarize the information read. In this summary, important information is included and predictable portions might be deleted. Sixth, and finally, a schema, along' with the recalled specific text information, enables the reader to postulate any missing information. For example, if someone wants to recall the type of drink that was mentioned in a text as served with a certain meal, and if the person can remember the meal, that person can guess what type of drink is usually served with that meal. In light of these schema theory principles, a body of research. has been. conducted to investigate the extent to which schema theory can help readers readily ‘ comprehend texts. In the following sections, schema theory and readings in L1 and schema theory and L2 are reviewed. Following that is a review of the literature abc lit wri has rea hav: (19E voce text subj asso subj The they info knowj know] reCa] P°Str antic Pest 14 about prior knowledge and writing. Finally, the literature regarding how students' prior knowledge and writing performance are measured is reviewed. Schema Theory and L1 Reading In the past decade, a large amount of research has been conducted to investigate how native English readers significantly comprehemd a text for which they have high background knowledge. Langer and Nicolich (1981) examined how the text's specific concept and vocabulary affect the readers' processing and recalling a text. Their subjects were 36 high school seniors. The subjects' knowledge was tested by asking them to write associations to the key words of two passages. Then the subjects were given the two passages to read silently. The researchers then asked the subjects to write, what they could remember about the passages. When the information recalled by the subjects was compared to the knowledge they had, it was found that the level of prior (nowledge was strongly related to the information :ecalled from the passage. In related studies, Pearson et al. (1979) :ostulated that understanding, retelling, and nticipating structures depend on elaborate schemata and ast experience. These authors found that comprehending text is a process of integrating text information with preex were intng possii how fa fifth- differ was r Conver reader Taylor succeg the te: who int their 5 deficie Poor re between memory the ct} related comprehe that t 15 preexisting schemata. They added that if the schemata were not strong, comprehension would be low because the integration of new information with schemata would not be possible. Taylor (1979) conducted a study to investigate how familiar texts aided the comprehension of third— and fifth-grade children. Taylor concluded that the difference between poor and good readers' comprehension was not significant when the texts were familiar. Conversely, the difference between those two types of readers was significant when the texts were unfamiliar. Taylor suggested that both good and poor readers successfully utilize the top—down processing Inode when the text is familiar. Contrasting results were provided by Eamon (1979) who indicated that poor readers were not able to utilize their schemata to comprehend a text. The reason for this deficiency among poor readers as stated by Eamon was that poor readers did not have the ability to differentiate >etween related and unrelated information during the remory search for relevant information. Good readers, on :he other hand, were able to locate easily the text— elated information in their minds and utilize it to omprehend that text. The implication of this study was hat the role of prior knowledge should not be Jerestimated. Reading teachers must make sure that less prc act rea for imp tex the com sin corn rea res fol rol the km 461 38d the} EXaI 16 proficient readers get the assistance they need for activating the schemata related to the text they are reading. This research illustrates how important schema is for native speakers' reading comprehension. This importance stems from the fact that schema theory gives an important role to the comprehender as well as the text. Unless successful interaction takes place between the comprehender's knowledge and the text's knowledge, comprehension will be incomplete. It was assumed that since prior knowledge affects native speakers' reading comprehension, it also affects nonactive speakers' reading comprehension. Therefore, ESL reading researchers started to investigate this assumption. The following section reviews the studies investigating the role of prior knowledge in L2 reading comprehension in the light of schema theory. Schema Theory and L2 Reading Content Schema Content schema was defined by Carrell (1987) as nowledge relative to the content domain of the text (p. 61). A number of studies have been conducted with econd-language learners to answer the question of how hey use content schemata to comprehend a passage. xamining the effects of background knowledge on native and stu fir or tra tex fan the tha tak tex sam of rep pre bot lan rec Lee tha com: fee. was red 17 and nonnative speakers' comprehension, Carrell (1983) studied three components of background knowledge: the first one was context; presence or absence of a picture or a page title before the page. Second was transparency: presence of lexical items which provide textual clues. The third background component was familiarity' with. the text or being’ knowledgeable about the text to be read. The findings of this study showed that unlike native speakers, nonnative speakers did not take advantage of their prior knowledge to comprehend a text. Lee (1986) replicated Carrell's study using the same components of background knowledge as Carrell. One of the differences between Carrell's study and Lee's replication of Carrell's study was that the instructions preceding the passages to be read in Lee's studies were both written and read aloud in the subjects' native language. Also, subjects were asked to write their recollections in their native language. The findings of Lee's study contradicted those of Carrell. Lee found that nonnative speakers were able to utilize the combination of their background knowledge components to recall the text. The reason Lee differed from Carrell was that Lee asked his subjects to write their recollections in their native language which gave them an adv; (19: was adva text Cont addi ESL SChe orga form DOSE inte Comp text Carr and fami 18 advantage in demonstrating their comprehension. Lee (1986) said: . . . the way in which comprehension is assessed affects the evaluation of comprehension. Assessing comprehension. with. a target language task may limit learners' ability to demonstrate what they comprehended. Assessing comprehension with the native language allows learners to more fully demonstrate their comprehension (1986, p. 353). Commenting on this matter, Swaffar (1988) said it was obvious that using a native language provided advantages over using the second language in recalling a text. Content Schema vs. Formal Schema Reading theorists have indicated that, in ‘addition tr) content schema, formal schema also affected ESL reading comprehension. Carrell (1987) defined formal schema as "knowledge relative to the formal rhetorical organizational structures of different types" (p. 461). In comparing the effects of content schema and formal schema on reading comprehension, Carrell (1987) posed the questions: what is the effect of the interactions of content and formal schema on a text comprehension? And what is the effect of one schema on a text comprehension when the other is held constant? Carrell had her intermediate ESL subjects read, recall, and ranswer questions about two texts. One text was Familiar and 111 a well—organizational rhetorical format. The uno wit comj unf Fur witi for] tru. COH‘ HOWT fam; imp< SChE rtad abSe 19 The other text was unfamiliar and rhetorically unorganized. Carrell's findings showed that the text with familiar content and familiar form was easy to comprehend; the text with unfamiliar content and unfamiliar form was difficult to comprehend. Furthermore, this study's findings showed that a text with familiar content, but with unfamiliar rhetorical form, was also easy to comprehend. The opposite was not true. That is, if the text's form was familiar, but its content was unfamiliar, comprehension was difficult. So, content familiarity had a more important role in the comprehension of unfamiliar content than did form. However, the form familiarity did a job that content familiarity could not do. That form familiarity was more important in "the comprehension of the top—level episodic structure of a 'text and in 'the comprehension of event sequences and temporal relationships among events" (Carrell, 1987, p. 476). Therefore, each type of schema content and form appears to play an important, but unique role in comprehension. ultural Schema As indicated by Carrell (1987), cultural content chema was powerful to the extent that it facilitated the eader's reading comprehension and compensated for the bsence of formal schema. Carrell said: "Whether the for sig bas cul The Ind cus the the suh mom the the Wen difl imp: Sche fast alth demc SUCc fr0n cUlt effe 20 form was rhetorically familiar or unfamiliar had no significant effect on subjects engaging in culturally based elaborations or distortions" (p. 474). Steffenson et al. (1979) studied how different cultural background knowledge assisted comprehension. Their subjects, native English speakers and native Indians, were asked to read passages about wedding customs in their cultures and in different cultures, and then recall, in writing, information about the passages they had read. The result of this study showed that subjects were able to read faster and to remember details more accurately, and make more accurate inferences when they read about their own wedding customs. In addition, the readers' recollections revealed that wrong inferences were obtained as a result of reading passages from different cultures. These findings indicated two important points. First, readers with an elaborate schema take advantage of that schema to read successfully faster, remember better, and infer more details. Second, although native English speakers and native Indians demonstrated high. language proficiency, they could not successfully remember details or make accurate inferences from passages about cultures other than their own :ulture. In a related study, Johnson (1981) examined the affect of interaction of linguistic complexity (vor cult Ame] Her that knor groi infr M on emp] pro: matr stur tOp; Shi: rta< (19: indi Com This beti SChs 21 (vocabulary difficulty and length of sentences) and cultural background knowledge. Johnson asked Iranian and American subjects to read passages from both cultures. Her findings were consistent with those of Steffenson: that cultural background had more effect than linguistic knowledge on comprehension. She also found that both groups were easily and quantitatively able to elicit information from culturally familiar passages. Schema Theory and Old Views of Reading The traditional way of reading put the emphasis on processing language form and structure. It also emphasized reading correctly, tied reading with language proficiency, ignored the values of students (by assigning materials that may have ignored the values of the students), and focused on mechanics and cohesion. The topic to be read was usually out of context. With schema theory, however, the emphasis has shifted from reading as a ineans to learn structure to reading for meaning. The meaning of a text as Fish (1980) and Iser (1978) indicated does not have an independent existence. Rather, it developes in the context of the interaction between readers and texts. This interaction occurs as top-down processing and Dottom—up processing take place, simultaneously. In schema theory, one processing mode cannot be substituted for the hav; WOL‘K prer bot ski (Ca: dif comj wri mg, The Sin aid bel. iHW 22 for the other. In top—down processing, readers overcome the problem of insufficient background knowledge by having slots to be filled during* reading. 111 other words, background knowledge enabled readers to make predictions that need to be confirmed during reading. In bottom—up processing, readers employ "language decoding skills, grammatical skills and vocabulary development" (Carrell, 1986, p. 8). These studies demonstrated the importance of different types of prior knowledge in reading comprehension. 1 Prior Knowledge and Writing Among Native Speakers The presumed similarity between reading and writing rests on the fact that reading and writing are cognitive processes aimed at constructing meaning. Therefore, writing researchers have hypothesized that since prior knowledge aids reading comprehension, it also aids writing performance. Chesky (1984) says: If reading, originally thought to be a simple decoding process, is heavily dependent on prior knowledge, then it is highly probably that writing, possibly a cognitively more demanding encoding process, is even. more dependent on prior knowledge (p. 8). Writing’ researchers with the guidance of’ this elief started to conduct studies for the sake of nvestigating the relationship between. prior 'knowledge and wit.‘ stur ess; semi knor Lani bet' knor bet‘ the mom exa Cohl Cre.‘ Stlh t0b: Tea: and eSsa the 685E and knot 23 and writing. All of the studies so far have been done with native speakers. Langer (1984) asked 99 tenth—grade students in four American history classes to write two essays at two different periods of time during the semester. Prior to that, Langer measured the students' knowledge of the topics about which they would write. Langer found papers requiring comparison or contrast were better written by students who possessed organized knowledge. Students who had fluent information wrote better argumentative papers. On the whole, Langer found that students with high, knowledge wrote quantitatively more and qualitatively better. In a similar study, Chesky and Hiebert (1987) examined the influence of high and low knowledge on coherence, syntactic complexity, essay length, content creating context, and writing errors. They asked 80 students in the 11th grade to write about two topics: tobacco price supports and school instruction. After measuring the students' prior knowledge of those topics and after scoring the coherence, syntactic complexity, essay length, context creating statements, and errors in the students' writing, correlation analysis was used to assess the relationship between these writing' measures and the students' prior knowledge. The findings of this study showed that prior :nowledge significantly correlated with the students' holi know with know them stud leng of t thos (309 know show make disc unde 24 holistic scores. In other words, students with high knowledge had higher holistic scores than the students with low knowledge. The students with high prior knowledge were also able to organize their ideas and make them into an integrated whole. Chesky and Hiebert's study also showed that prior knowledge affected the length of the students' essays. They said: "The letters of the high-prior knowledge group were twice as long as those of the students in the low—prior knowledge group" (309). Concerning the relationship between prior knowledge and context—creating statements, this study showed that students with prior knowledge were able to make an outline which served to organize the ideas to be discussed and to create a context which facilitated the understanding of those ideas. In surveying the 80 students involved in the study for investigating the effect of prior knowledge on the students' attitudes toward the topic, Chesky and Hiebert found that those with high knowledge liked the topic and were more involved in it. This study showed no relationship between prior knowledge and T—unit length and writing errors (punctuation, capitalization, spelling, grammar, and usage). To account for this finding, Chesky and Hiebert suggested that the existence 3f, or lack of, grammatical errors was not affected by :he students' prior knowledge because those errors may be wri dra sec Con bas abl act Dur fils. rel. Wit} 25 a function of the writers, themselves, and not of what they knew. DeGroff (1986) extended Langer‘s (1984) and Chesky and Hiebert's (1987) studies by examining the influence of elementary school students' prior knowledge of baseball on conferencing with peers about their first drafts and revising those drafts of a narrative. Forty students took tests originated by Graves (1983) and Calin (1983) to determine the level of conferencing behavior. Those 40 students also took a 49-item short answer test of baseball knowledge used in Mosenthal (1984) and Mosenthal et al. (1985) to determine the students' background knowledge. The students were then asked to write stories about a half inning of a baseball game. Analyzing students' performance on the first draft, conferencing with peers, and their revising of the second drafts, DeGroff found the students' final draft content showed the influence of prior knowledge of baseball. That is, students with high knowledge were able to provide more propositions about "an auxiliary action," one of the grammatical baseball categories. During conferences, students with high knowledge were also able to make comments on specific content features related directly to the goals of the game, while those with low knowledge made only general comments not related to baseball goals. Regarding the second draft, this str cha con inf pri McC sut con knc stu coh abl mor (19 USE inc ill abi The 26 study showed that students with high knowledge made more changes than the students with low knowledge. The conclusion of this study was that prior knowledge influenced the students' writing processes. Another interesting study about the effect of prior knowledge on writing coherence ‘was conducted by McCutchen (1986). McCutchen first tested her 30 subjects' knowledge of football by giving them 30 questions about a football game. Then she asked them to write compositions about football. After the coherence of those compositions was evaluated, McCutchen found that composition coherence was correlated with the amount of knowledge the students had about football. That is, students with high knowledge were able to generate coherent texts. Furthermore, those students were also able to bring out and discuss important ideas of football more coherently than the students with low knowledge. To have successful written communication, Thomas (1986) asserts three types of knowledge are crucial: 1. Knowledge of conventions which includes the use of words and grammar. 2. Knowledge of language, or discourse which includes the three speech acts: perlocutionary, illocutionary, and locutionary acts. This means the ability of the writer to do something to the audience. The writer may want to move the audience, amuse them, knc int nee eas Cal man the (19 wel she the mat. 27 inform them, or persuade them to perform the perlocutionary speech act. When stating these goals, a writer' may want to advise, assert, argue, promise, or assess to perform the illocutionary speech act. Writers will refer to entities, such as places, things, or ideas to express their intentions while performing the locutionary speech act. 3. Knowledge of the world which means that writers should have a good sense of what their audience knows. Thomas says: a skilled writer will have a fairly accurate idea of what she can expect her audience to know about the words—-facts, common opinions, and so forth. World knowledge is a good understanding of what her audience already knows and believes about the world (p. 587). To do this, writers should provide only required information without saying more or less than what is needed. They also should provide true, relevant, and easy—to-understand information. Those four rnaxims are :alled the maxims of quantity, quality, relation, and nanner, previous experience and writing. In suggesting :he experiential approach to teaching writing, Judy 1980) says: "to write well, one must know something ell" (p. 39). Judy indicates that composition teachers hould rely on the students' experience and what they ow in order for the students to produce better written terials. Students, however, Judy says, should be given an WIS res (l! as: ati tin wit ple Kel wit gr: in and tilt 28 an opportunity to talk about what they know before writing. He calls this technique an experience reexamination. Chiesi et al. (1979), Hilgers (1982), and Kellogg (1987) indicate that the reason prior topic knowledge is associated with better writing is that students pay more attention to their writing instead of spending effort and time generating ideas. Kellogg indicated that students with high knowledge demonstrated less cognitive effort to plan, organize, and review their ideas. Agreeing with Kellogg, Hilgers stated that if the writers are at ease with the content, they will give arrangement, style, grammar, and inechanics more attention which will result ‘in improvement of the quality of their writing. Writing and Topic Choice Kincaid (1953) hypothesized that the quality of students' writing is stable regardless of the topic on hich a student writes. Kincaid found out that the ollege students' writing quality varied according to the ssigned topic, and so his hypothesis was rejected. ater, Wiseman and Wrigley (1958), in a similar study, oncluded that the differences found in the children's riting were caused by the titles of the topics. Wiseman nd Wrigley suggested that writing teachers should employ lternative title techniques when assigning an essay paper inten knowl :2 s2.- p—I rt- mm rat—'0 ... moyp .. 0 am (... E? a) <: might Stude there infor activ the ; McKay 10Urn retri iflStr 29 paper. In this way, a student could choose a subject of interest. McKay (1984) indicated that limited topic knowledge may impede good writing, . . we can allow students a great deal of choice in the selection of writing topics. If we want to select only one topic, we should choose a topic about which the students are likely to have knowledge or experience. Therefore, topics which presuppose a great deal of specific knowledge about American culture or about a particular academic field with which the students are unfamiliar should be avoided. For example, an assignment which asks students to compare and contrast the educational system of their native country with that of the United States assumes that the students know a great deal about the American system of education. If they do not have this knowledge, they will have little or nothing to say (p. 189). To avoid this problem, McKay said composition instructors might need to spend some class time "familiarizing" the students with the topic through reading and discussion. Since, as Flower and Hayes (1981) indicated, there can be a problem in retrieving a suitable amount of information relevant to academic writing, McKay suggested activating the students' schemata of any topic, even if the students happened to know' a great deal about it. McKay recommended heuristic devices, brainstorming, or journal writing as techniques which might help students retrieve amount of apt information. Calkins (1983) indicated that successful writing instruction allows children either to choose their own top. bet' chi. abor (19: Stat sco: ass: rea: Shor tea: Prir EXM meci the} the ask film: 30 topics or to employ brainstorming techniques as an aid to better writing. Graves (1983) also contends that a children's writing quality is better when they write about something they know: Writers who learn to choose topics well make the most significant growth in both information and skills at the point of best topic. With best topic, the child exercises strongest control, establishes ownership, and with ownership, pride in the piece (p. 21). These findings are consistent with Hoetker's (1982) contention that when the scores of California State University and College Equivalency Examinations' scores in 1973 and 1974 declined drastically, the topics assigned :hi the two examinations were reviewed and the reason for the variations in scores was found to be due to the topics assigned for the examination. The review shows the 1974 topic "called for highly abstract reasoning and was manifestly more difficult than the prior year's topic which called upon a personal experience" (p. 381). Hoetker concluded that the dull, mechanical, and abstract pieces of text came as a result of asking the students to write on topics about which they knew little or nothing. The implication of ioetker's report is that teachers and examiners should ask students to write about topics with which they are familiar. pr: thr (l! 31 Language Proficiency and Writing Among Nonnative Speakers In reading, both prior knowledge and language competence are important for the interaction of both top- down and bottom—up processing modes. In discussing reading comprehension, we have seen that without this interaction, reading comprehension will be incomplete. So far, all the studies which investigated the effects of prior knowledge on writing revealed that prior knowledge is also crucial for writing. Concerning language proficiency in relation with writing, Raimes (1987) suggests that decisions regarding the placement of students in writing courses must not depend solely O monoom mocoflOEMOHm msflufluz can ompmHBOsM soflnm soospom soflpmHoHMOU .m manna 75 fluency (.357, p<.01), organization (.457, p<.001), and combination (.494, p<.001). For the Arabic subgroup, there was a positive significant relationship between overall prior knowledge and writing proficiency scores (.767, p<.05). There was also a positive significant relationship between writing proficiency scores and fluency (.748, p<.05), organization (.80, p<.05), and combination (.732, p<.05). For the Chinese subgroup, there was a positive Significant relationship only between fluency and writing proficiency scores (.612, p<.05). For the Japanese subgroups, there was a positive Significant relationship between overall prior knowledge and writing proficiency scores (.755, p<.001). There was also a positive significant relationship between writing proficiency scores and fluency (.363, p<.05), Organization (.730, p<.001), and combination (.772, p<.001). For the miscellaneous group, there was a positive Significant relationship only between fluency and writing proficiency scores (.696, p<.01). On the other hand, looking at the same table, we conclude that there was not any significant relationship between prior knowledge as a total and writing prOficiency scores for Chinese and miscellaneous subgroups, between organization and writing proficiency 76 scores for Chinese and miscellaneous subgroup, or between combination and writing proficiency scores for Chinese and miscellaneous subgroups. Writing Proficiency Scores and Overall Language Proficiency Hypothesis 1.B: There is no significant relationship between overall language proficiency and writing proficiency scores. Table 10 shows that there was a positive significant relationship between overall language proficiency and global language proficiency (.333, p<.01) for all the group. Table 10. Correlation Between Overall Language Proficiency (OLP) and Writing Proficiency Scores Groups OLP/Writing Global Proficiency All Groups .333** Arabic .279 Chinese .289 Japanese .335* Miscellaneous .401 *p < .05 **p < .01 77 For the Japanese subgroup, there was also a positive significant relationship between overall language proficiency and writing proficiency scores (.335, p<.05). On the other hand, looking at the same table, we find no relationship between overall language proficiency and writing proficiency scores for the Arabic, the Chinese, and the miscellaneous subgroups. The Additive Effect The question related to the additive effect is: What is the additive effect of prior knowledge and overall language proficiency on writing proficiency scores? The results of using regression analysis indicated in Table 11 show that prior knowledge was a Significant predictor for writing proficiency scores at the p<.01 level. The results also show that 29.2% of the total variance in writing proficiency scores was accounted for by both variables. Stepwise regression analysis show that 23.5% of the total variance in the writing proficiency scores was accounted for by the prior knowledge alone. The overall language proficiency, however, is a marginal predictor for the writing proficiency scores at the p<.0591) level. So, the results of the relationship between prior knowledge and 78 Table 11. Regression Results of Writing Proficiency Scores (n = 49) Variables Coefficient Significance Overall Knowledge .2968 .0013* Language Proficiency .1579 .0591 Constant 56.7116 .0000** Multiple R = .5408 R-SQR = .2925 *p < .01; **p < .001 writing proficiency scores show that there was a significant positive relationship between all prior knowledge scores and writing proficiency scores for almost all the group and all the subgroups. The very few instances where there is lack of significant relationship between prior knowledge and writing proficiency scores do not keep us from concluding that the relationship between prior knowledge and writing proficiency scores is significant. Concerning the results of the relationship between overall language proficiency and writing proficiency scores, we find that they were significantly related for the whole group and for the Japanese subgroup only. 79 However, prior knowledge accounts for more variance on the writing proficiency scores than overall language proficiency. Content: Number of Words and Number of Ideas Content and Prior Knowledge Hypothesis 1.B: There is no relationship between the nonnative speakers' prior knowledge and the content: number of words and number of ideas. Number of Words Table 12 shows that there was a positive significant relationship between overall prior knowledge and the number of words (.406, p<.01) for all the groups. There was also a positive significant relationship between the number of words and fluency (.410, p<.01), organization (387, p<.01), and combination (.387, p<.01) for all the groups. For the Arabic group, there was a positive significant relationship between overall prior knowledge and the number of words (.694, p<.05). There was also a positive significant relationship between the number of words and organization (.720, p<.05), and combination (.676, p<.05) for the Arabic subgroup. For the Chinese subgroup, there was a positive significant relationship between prior knowledge and the number of words (.719, p<.05). There was also a positive 8O HOO. v asks HO. v firs mo. v a. w¥mmh. NQH. xsmwh. VON. ixowh. NVN. kxhow. HaN. mSOwCMHHOUmHZ «sovm. «mow. rimmv. «hmm. MNN. «#QHV. «*NNm. «hmv. OWOCQQMU «mow. i«mmh. smnm. *«QHN. ««Hmw. «VNQ. «mam. «mHh. mmOCHLU «NHQ. «WPQ. sromw. «ONF. smmh. Omw. immm. «v00. UflQflH4 «ssmmm. 445mm. «*«hom. «sham. «soflv. ¥«OH¢. «irmvm. *xoov. QDOHO Had mmmpH mpuoz mmwvH mpuoz mmoUH mpuoz mmopH mpuog msouo coflHMCHQEOU COflUMNHcmmHO kocosam mmpoa3ocM Hfimum>o mmmpH m0 qu532 pcm mono: Mo quEsz msouo :omm can mmsouo Ham now .ucoucoo pcm ompmazocx Hoaum coozuom cowumHoHHoo .NH manna 81 significant relationship between the number of words and fluency (.624, p<.05), organization (.716, p<.05), and combination (.728, p<.01) for the Chinese subgroup. For the Japanese subgroup, there was a significant positive relationship between prior knowledge and the number of words (.427, p<.05). There was also a significant positive relationship between the number of words and fluency (.416, p<.01), organization (.387, p<.05), and combination (.403, p<.05) for the Japanese subgroup. On the other hand, looking at Table 12 we find that there was not any significant relationship between fluency and the number of words for the Arabic subgroup. We also found no significant relationship between prior knowledge as total or as components and the number of words for the miscellaneous subgroup. The Number of Ideas Table 12 also shows that there was a significant positive relationship between overall prior knowledge and the number of ideas (.542, p<.001) for all the group. There was also a positive significant relationship between the number of ideas and fluency (.410, p<.01), organization (.507, p<.001), and combination (.553, p<.001) for all the group. 82 Table 13. Correlation Between Overall Language Proficiency (OLP) and Content: Number of Words and Number of Ideas Groups OLP/Content Number of Words Number of Ideas All Groups .298* .166 Arabic .284 .629 Chinese .776** .439 Japanese .348* .177 Miscellaneous .201 .250 *p <.05 **p < .001 For the Arabic subgroup, there was a significant positive relationship between overall prior knowledge and the number of ideas (.828, p<.05). There was also a positive significant relationship between the number of ideas and fluenCy (.759, p<-05). organization (.850, p<.01), and combination (.812, p<.05) for the Arabic subgroup. For the Chinese subgroup, there was a positive significant relationship between overall prior knowledge and the number of ideas (.519, p<.05). There was also a significant relationship between the number of ideas and 83 fluency (.731, p<.01), organization (.573, p<.05), and combination (.498, p<.05) for the Chinese subgroup. For the Japanese subgroup, there was a positive significant relationship between overall prior knowledge and the number of ideas (.522, p<.01). There was also a positive significant relationship between the number of ideas and organization (.495, p<.01), and combination (.546, p<.01) for the Japanese subgroup. For the miscellaneous subgroup, there was a positive significant relationship between overall prior knowledge and the number of ideas (.807, p.<01). There was also a positive significant relationship between the number of ideas and fluency (.760, p<.01), organization (.763, p<.01), and combination (.785, p<.01) for the miscellaneous subgroup. On the other hand, looking at Table 12, we found that there was not a significant relationship between fluency and the number of ideas only in the Japanese subgroup. Content and Overall Language Proficiency Hypothesis 2—B: There is no relationship between overall language proficiency and content: number of words and number of ideas. Number of Words Table 13 shows that there was a positive significant relationship between overall language 84 proficiency and number of words (.298, p<.05) for all the groups. There was also a positive significant relationship between overall language proficiency and number of words (.776, p<.01) for the Chinese subgroup. A positive significant relationship also existed between overall language proficiency and number of words (.348, p<.05) for the Japanese subgroup. On the other hand, there was no significant relationship between overall language proficiency and the number of words for the Arabic subgroup, and the miscellaneous subgroup. The Number of Ideas Table 13 shows that there was no significant relationship between overall language proficiency and the number of ideas for all the group or for the subgroups. The Additive Effect The question related to the additive effect is: What is the additive effect of prior knowledge and overall language proficiency on content: number of words and number of ideas? The results of regression indicated in Table 14 for the number of words show that overall knowledge is a significant predictor for the number of words at p<.01, while the overall language proficiency is not. Both variables account for 21.2% of the total variance in the 85 Table 14. Regression Results of the Content: Number of Words (n = 49) Variables Coefficient Significance Overall Knowledge 6.1842 .0098** Language Proficiency 3.6465 .0993 Constant —.0011 .3717 Multiple R = .4614 R—SQR = .2129 **p < .01. number of words. A stepwise regression analysis shows that prior knowledge alone accounts for 16.4% of the total variance in the number of words. The results of regression indicated in Table 15 for the number of ideas show that the overall prior knowledge as a significant predictor for the number of ideas at p<.001. Both variables account for 29.7% of the total variance in the number of ideas. A stepwise regression analysis shows that the overall prior knowledge variable alone accounts for 29.4% of the total variance ill the number of ideas. The overall language proficiency was not a significant predictor for the number of ideas. So, the results of the relationship between prior knowledge and content indicate that prior knowledge had a 86 Table 15. Regression Results of the Content: Number of Ideas (n = 24) Variables Coefficient Significance Overall Knowledge .0726 .0001*** Language Proficiency .0075 .6473 Constant Multiple R = .5451 R-SQR = .2971 ***p < .01 strong positive relationship with content: number of words and number of ideas. The overall prior knowledge was also found to be a significant predictor for both number of words and number of ideas. On the other hand, the overall language proficiency had no relationship with content. It was not a significant predictor for content either. Text Sophistication Text sophistication and prior knowledge: Hypothesis 1-C: There is no relationship between the nonnative speakers' prior knowledge and text sophistication. Table 16 illustrates that there was a pmsitive significant relationship between overall knowledge and text sophistication (.283, p<.05) for the whole groups. There was also a positive significant relationship shown 87 Ho. v Q44 VQ¥ mo. «mmw. «mam. ««mmn. *Hmm. msoocmHHoomflz SN. mom. mmo. SN. omocmaae NHq. omv.| HHa.1 smm.| omocHLU mmw. mmm. smmw. qmm. oflhmud Lam. 2.5.. mam. «SN. 3:05 2a cofluoOHumHsQom :oHumOHpmflcmom :oflumoflumflsmom coflpmoflumflsmwm uxoe uxoe uxoe u H masono \coHumcH Eou coaummacmmuo \xoco: \ompoa30cx . .n \ . . Hm Hamuo>o How cofluMOHumflsmom uxoe pom mmpoazocx uoHum cooBuom COHumaouuou dsouo comm ocm mmoouo HH< .wH THEME 88 between text sophistication and organization (.271, p<.05) and combination (.281, p<.05) for all the groups. For the Arabic subgroup there was a positive significant relationship between fluency and text sophistication (.689, p<.05). For the miscellaneous subgroup, there was a positive significant relationship between overall prior knowledge and text sophistication (.661, p<.05). There was also a positive significant relationship between text sophistication and fluency (.729, p<.01), organization (.617, p<.05), and combination (.626, p<.05). Looking at Table 16, we see that there was no significant relationship between fluency and text sophistication for all the groups. For the Arabic subgroup, there was no significant relationship between text sophistication and overall knowledge, organization, or combination. For the Chinese and the Japanese subgroups, there was no significant relationship between prior knowledge total or any components with text sophistication. 89 Text Sophistication and Overall Language Proficiency Hypothesis 2—C: There is no relationship between overall language proficiency and text sophistication of the nonnative speakers' written discourse. Table 17 shows that there was a positive significant relationship between overall language proficiency and text sophistication (.394, p<.05) for the whole group. Table 17. Correlation Between Overall Language Proficiency and Text Sophistication Groups OLP/Text Sophistication All Groups .394* Arabic .863** Chinese —.633* Japanese .098 Miscellaneous .137 *p < .05 **p < .01 For the Arabic subgroup, there was also a positive significant relationship between overall language proficiency and text sophistication (.863, p<.01). However, for the Chinese subgroup, there was a negative significant relationship between overall 90 language proficiency and text sophistication (—.633, p<.05). On the other hand, for the Japanese and the miscellaneous subgroups, there was no significant relationship between overall language proficiency and text sophistication. The Additive Effect The question related to the additive effect is: what is the additive effect of prior knowledge and overall language proficiency on text sophistication? The results of regression as indicated in Table 18 show that overall knowledge is marginally related to text sophistication at p<.0528. Looking at the same table, we notice that 8.05% of the total variance on the text sophistication was accounted for by overall knowledge. The overall language proficiency does not significantly contribute to the prediction of text sophistication. 91 Table 18. Regression Results of Text Sophistication Variables Coefficient Significance Overall Knowledge .0445 .0528 Language Proficiency .0420 .8979 Constant 3.2280 .0018** Multiple R = 2838 R-SQR = .0805 **p < .01. Also there was a positive significant relationship between overall language proficiency and text sophistication for all the groups, and the Arabic subgroup. For the Chinese subgroup, there was a negative significant relationship between overall language proficiency and text sophistication. The additive effect results show that overall knowledge must be considered a marginal predictor of the text sophistication. Global Coherence Global Coherence and Prior Knowledge Hypothesis 1—D: There is no significant relationship between the nonnative speakers' prior knowledge and global coherence of their essays. 92 Table 19 shows that there as a positive significant relationship between overall knowledge and global coherence (.273, p<.05) for all the group. There was also a positive significant relationship between global coherence and fluency (.307, p<.05) and combination (.268, p<.05) for all the group. This is also exactly the same for the Japanese There was a positive significant relationship subgroup. between overall knowledge and global coherence (.391, p<.05) for this subgroup. There was also a positive significant relationship between global coherence and fluency (.413, p<.05) and combination (.377, p<.05) for the Japanese subgroup. On the other hand, looking at the same table, we find that there was no significant relationship between organization and global coherence in all the groups. For the Arabic, Chinese, or miscellaneous subgroups, there was no significant relationship between global coherence and overall knowledge, or its three components. For the Japanese subgroup, only organization did not have a significant correlation with global coherence. Global Coherence and Overall Lppguage Proficiency Hypothesis 2—D: There is no significant relationship between overall language proficiency and global coherence in the nonnative speakers' written discourse. 93 mo. v as hem. Ham. Ham. «mm. msoocoaaoomflz seem. cam. emae. *Hmm. omocmmmb mva. NNH. mmfi. oma. omoCHsU owe. mom. 0mm. Ham. oanmud «mmm. mmm. .som. .msm. mnsouo sea mucouosoo mucouosoo oocouocou ooconocou \cofluoCHQEoo \cofiummflcmquo \mososam \omwwmwmmw mesouw msouo comm use mQSOHU Ham How mononosoo HoQon can ompoazocm Hoflhm coozbom coaumaonuoo .mH wanna 94 Table 20 shows that there was no significant relationship between overall language proficiency and global coherence the whole group, the Arabic, the Chinese, the Japanese, or the miscellaneous subgroups. Table 20. Correlation Between Overall Language Proficiency and Global Coherence OLP/Global Coherence Groups All Groups .099 Arabic —.316 Chinese .196 Japanese -.034 Miscellaneous .432 The Additive Effect The question related to the additive effect is: What is the additive effect of prior knowledge and overall language proficiency on global coherence? The results of regression indicated in Table 21 show that neither prior knowledge nor overall language proficiency were a significant predictor for global coherence. 95 Table 21. Regression Results of Global Coherence (n = 49) Variables Coefficient Significance Overall Knowledge .0144 .0753 Language Proficiency .0136 .7509 Constant 1.0460 .0209 Multiple R = .2763 R-SQR = .0763 So, the results of the relationship between prior knowledge and global coherence show that these two variables are significantly related for all the groups and for the Japanese subgroups with the exception of organization. The prior knowledge and global coherence are not significantly related for the Arabic, the Chinese, and the miscellaneous subgroups. Overall language proficiency, on the other hand, did not have a significant relationship with global coherence for all the groups or with any of the subgroups. None of the prior knowledge or overall language proficiency scores was a significant predictor for global coherence. 96 Linguistic Complexity: T—Unit Length and Subordination Linguistic Complexity and Prior Knowledge Hypothesis 1-E: There is no relationship between the nonnative speakers' prior knowledge and T—unit length and subordination in their essays. Table 22 shows that there was a positive, significant relationship between overall prior knowledge score and T-unit length only for the Chinese subgroup (.662, p<.05). Also, the prior knowledge are broken down into the three components of fluency, organization, and combination there is a statistical significant relationship between each component with T—unit length. That is, T—unit length is significantly correlated with fluency (.776, p<.01), organization (.611, p><.05), and combination (.633, p<.05). There is also a statistically significant relationship between organization and T—unit length (.282, p<.05) only for the whole group. There is also a negative significant relationship between prior knowledge and subordination (-.819, p.<05) in the Arabic subgroup. On the other hand, looking at the same table we can conclude that there was not any statistical Significant relationship between the two components of linguistic complexity and the overall knowledge as a total or the prior knowledge components for all the group or for any of the subgroupS- 97' HO. v Qua mo. v a. Low. Ham. mmm. mam. woo. mam. mQN. wam. moomcmaaoomaz smo.s Ham. aqo.u mv~. coo. «06.- smo.u ova. omocmamo mam.- «mmo. Ham. .Hao. 6mm. 446ss. mam. «moo. mmmcaro .mam.- mma. ms~.- can. Hmo.n mmm. #65.) non. cabana was. mmm. ese. .mmm. ems. men. doe. mom. mosouo Ada :Oaumcflouonsm ”NWMWM sewum2wouonsm ”NWHWM :oflumcfipuonsm ”wmmwm :oHumsaouonsm ”HWMWN mm20u . o :anncaneoo :ofiumnacmmuo >ocosam ompoazocx aaouo>o can :uocoq uflcste msouo comm can masouo Ham uOu :OAumceouonsm u>nwxoameou oHumaso:MA use ompmazosx newum coozuom codenaouuou .NN OHQMB 98 Linguistic Complexity and Overall Language Proficiency Hypothesis 2-E: There is no significant relationship between overall language proficiency and linguistic complexity. Table 23 shows that there was a positive significant relationship between overall language Table 23. Correlation Between Overall Language Proficiency (OLP) and Linguistic Complexity Linguistic Complexity Groups OLP/T-unit Length OLP/Subordination All Groups .438*** .172 Arabic .136 —.792* Chinese -.021 -.513 Japanese .514* .406* Miscellaneous .491 .287 *p < .05 ***p < .001 proficiency scores and T—unit length (.438, p<.001) for all the groups. There is also a negative significant relationship between overall language proficiency and subordination (—.792, p<.05) for the Arabic subgroup. For the Japanese subgroup, there is a positive significant relationship between overall language 99 proficiency scores and T—unit length (.514, p<.05) and subordination (.406, p<.05). Looking at the same table, we find that there was not any relationship between overall language proficiency and T—unit length for the Arabic, the Chinese, and the miscellaneous subgroups. There was not any relationship between overall language proficiency and subordination for all the groups, the Chinese, thee miscellaneous subgroups either. The Additive Effect The question related to the additive effect is: What is the additive effect of prior knowledge and language proficiency on the linguistic complexity: T— unit length and subordination? The results of regression for T-unit length are indicated in Table 24. The table shows that overall language proficiency was a significant predictor for T- unit length at p<.01, while the overall knowledge is not. Regression results also show that 22.4% of the total variance in the T—unit length was accounted for by both variables. A stepwise regression analysis shows that 19.2% of the total variance in the T—unit length. was accounted for by the overall language proficiency alone. 100 Table 24. Regression Results of the T—Unit Length (N= 49) Variables Coefficient Significance Language Proficiency .1278 .0041** Overall Knowledge .0621 .1714 Constant -.5866 .8572 Multiple R = .4738 R—SQR = .2245 **Significant at p < .01. Table 25 shows that overall knowledge and overall language proficiency were not significant predictors for subordination. So, the results of the relationship between prior knowledge and linguistic complexity show that there was a significant relationship between prior knowledge as a whole and as components and T-unit length only for the Chinese subgroup. The results of the relationship between overall language proficiency and linguistic complexity show that there was a significant relationship between overall language proficiency and T—unit length for the whole group, and for the Japanese subgroup. There was also a significant relationship between overall language proficiency and subordination for the Japanese subgroup, 101 Table 25. Regression Results of Subordination (N= 49) Variables Coefficient Significance Overall Knowledge .0058 .3267 Language Proficiency .0061 .3753 Constant 1.0060 .0390 Multiple R = .2152 R-SQR = .0436 but a significant negative relationship for the Arabic subgroup. The results of the effect of the interaction between prior knowledge and overall language proficiency on linguistic complexity show that only overall language proficiency was considered to be a significant predictor for T—unit length. Revision Strategies Revision Strategies and Prior Knowledge Hypothesis 1-F: There is no relationship between the nonnative speakers' prior knowledge and the number of revision strategies found in their essays. Table 26 shows that there was a positive significant relationship between revision strategies and 102 .mo. v a. whm. Hmm. mom. 0mm. msoocoaaoomflz ooo. omo. mos. ooo. mmmcmomo ofia.- mos.- owe.) mos. mmocaao loos. «moo. mom. Hos. oenmua oos. hos. ono. one. mosouo Ana cofimfl>om coflmfl>om soflmfl>om :oflmfi>om Hobos Hobos Hobos m Hobos ozone \oOaumoanaoo \coanmuncmono \soowsnn \6 amazocx HHMHo>O Ham now moflmmuonum coamfl>om can msouu comm pom masouw ompoazocx Hoflum cooKuom soaumaouuoo .mm oases 103 organization (.769, p<.05), and combination (.740, p<.05) for the Arabic subgroup. No other significant relationship was found between prior knowledge as total or between the prior knowledge components and revision strategies for all the groups or for any other subgroup. Revision Strategies and Overall Language Proficiency Hypothesis 2—F: There is no relationship between overall language proficiency and revision strategies. Table 27 shows that there was a significant positive relationship between overall language proficiency and revision strategies (.873, p<.01) for the Arabic subgroup only. Table 27. Correlation Between Overall Language . Proficiency (OLP) and Revision Strategies Groups OLP/Revision Strategies All Groups .122 Arabic .873** Chinese .439 Japanese -.017 Miscellaneous .212 **p < .01 104 No other significant relationship was found between overall language proficiency and revision strategies for all the group or for any other subgroup. The Additive Effect The question related to the additive effect is: What is the additive effect of prior knowledge and overall language proficiency on revision strategies? The results of regression indicated in Table 28 shows that neither prior knowledge nor overall language proficiency were a significant predictor for revision strategies. So, the results of the relationship between prior knowledge and revision strategies show that there was a positive significant relationship between revision strategies and organization and combination for the Arabic subgroup. The results of the relationship between overall language proficiency and revision strategies show that there was a positive significant relationship between revision strategies and overall language proficiency only for the Arabic subgroup. Neither prior knowledge nor overall language proficiency are significant predictors for revision strategies. 105 Table 28. Regression Results of Revision Strategies (n = 9) Variables Coefficient Significance Overall Knowledge .3928 .3101 Language Proficiency .2249 .5370 Constant —9.1572 .7427 Multiple R = .1925 R-SQR = .0370 Involvement Involvement and Prior Knowledge Hypothesis 1-G: There is no significant relationships between the nonnative speakers' prior knowledge and their involvement to writing. Table 29 shows that there was a positive significant relationship between overall knowledge and involvement (.272, p<.05) for the whole group. There is also a positive significant relationship between involvement and organization (.264, p<.05) and combination (.274, p<.05) for all the groups. Looking at the same table we notice that there was no significant relationship between prior knowledge as a whole or as components and involvement for any subgroup. 106 mo. v a. moo. Hem. mmo. eve. mooocmaaoOmHz oma. NNH. omo.- sag. mmmcmomo Hem. mam. mom. Nmm. omoCch HmH.I one.) omH.| mNH. mahnu< .osm. .oom. oma. .Nom. monouo Has usoEw>Ho>cH ucoeo>ao>sH bcoEo>Ho>cH ustoWHome \coflDMCHQEOO \coflbmuflcmmno \wocosHm \ Wawwo>w masonw moouo room can mmsomw Ham MOM usoEo>Ho>cH can ompoaaocm Hoosm coosuom GOHuMHoHMOU .mm wands 107 Involvement and Overall Language Proficiency Hypothesis 2—G: There is no relationship between overall language proficiency and involvement. Table 30 shows that there was no significant relationship between overall language proficiency and involvement for all the groups or for any of the subgroups. Table 30. Correlation Between Overall Language Proficiency and Involvement Groups Involvement All Groups .275 Arabic —.062 Chinese .485 Japanese .104 Miscellaneous —.141 The Additive Effect The question related to the additive effect is: What is the additive effect of overall prior knowledge and overall language proficiency on involvement. The results of' regression indicted in 'Table 31 show that neither overall knowledge nor overall language proficiency significantly contribute to the prediction of involvement. 108 Table 31. Regression Results of Involvement Variables .. Coefficient Significance Overall Knowledge .1950 .0734 Language Proficiency .0232 .8179 Constant 6.0294 .4384 Multiple R = 2740 R—SQR = .0751 So the results of the relationship between prior knowledge and involvement show that these two variables were not significantly related for all the groups with the exception of fluency. Fluency did not have a significant relationship with involvement for any of the groups. No other significant relationship was found between prior knowledge and involvement for any of the subgroups. The overall language proficiency also had no significant relationship with involvement. FUrthermore, neither prior knowledge nor overall language proficiency were a significant predictor for involvement. Summary Concerning the relationship between the demographic information (age, gender, and level of education) the results of the correlational analysis demonstrated a statistically significant relationship 109 between age and content and involvement and between level of education and involvement. The results also show a significant relationship between amount of reading and involvement. This means that the older the students, the more they write and the more they are involved with their writing. Also, the more they read, the more they were involved with their writing. Concerning the relationship between prior knowledge and writing measures, we conclude that there was a significant relationship between prior knowledge and writing proficiency scores and content. There was also a significant relationship between prior knowledge and text sophistication for all the groups and for the miscellaneous subgroup. The results also showed that there was a relationship between prior knowledge and global coherence, revision strategies, and involvement. The relationship between prior knowledge and T—unit length for the Chinese subgroup was significant, but weak for the rest of the subgroups. Finally, the relationship between prior knowledge and amount of subordination was not significant for the whole group and all of the subgroups. Concerning the relationship between overall language proficiency and writing, the results showed that there was a significant relationship between overall language proficiency and writing proficiency scores for 110 the whole group as a heterogeneous group, text sophistication for the whole group and for the Arabic subgroup, T—unit length for all the group and for the Japanese subgroup, subordination for the Japanese subgroup, and revision strategies for the Arabic subgroup. There was no significant relationship between overall language proficiency and involvement. With the exception of the T-unit length, prior knowledge had more effect on all the writing measures than overall language proficiency. It is also obvious from the results that prior knowledge accounted for more variance in writing than overall language proficiency did. Finally, the results also show that there was a difference among the participants' writing related to their native language. How that difference was caused by their native languages is beyond the scope of this study. CHAPTER V CONCLUSIONS Introduction This study, as discussed earlier, was designed to investigate the relationship among prior knowledge, overall language proficiency, and nonnative speakers' written discourse. Furthermore, this study has addressed several questions regarding the relationship among demographic information, amount of reading, the subjects' native language and writing measures. To review, the research questions regarding the nonnative speakers' written discourse were: 1. Is there any difference among the participants' writing related to their personal characteristics and the amount of reading they do? 2. What is the relationship between prior knowledge about specific topics and the nonnative speakers' written discourse? lll 112 3. What is the relationship between overall language proficiency and the nonnative speakers' written discourse? 4. What is the additive effect of prior knowledge and overall language proficiency on nonnative speakers' written discourse? Forty-nine advanced students attending levels D and E 111 the English Language Center at Michigan State University in Spring, 1988, wrote on the topic of Eclipses, a topic on which the students showed a large spread of knowledge. Before writing the essay, the participants took free-association and prior-knowledge tests on the topic. After writing the composition, they filled out a questionnaire, which reflected the degree of their involvement with the topic. Trained raters scored the participants' compositions on writing proficiency scores, content (the number of ideas and the number of words), text sophistication, global coherence, linguistic complexity (T-unit length and subordination), and revision strategies. The raters also scored the knowledge test for the degree of fluency and organization. Their background information, amount of reading, language proficiency scores, and involvement were self-reported on a questionnaire. Composition ratings, and responses to the questionnaire were 113 correlated using the Pearson—Product Moment Correlation coefficient. Regression analysis was used to quantify the additive effect between prior knowledge and overall language proficiency on writing measures. MANOVA, ANOVA, and Tukey Test were used to compare the subgroups' writing scores. Chapter IV presents the results in detail. Personal Characteristics and Writing 1. Is there any difference among the participants' writing related to their personal characteristics and the amount of reading they do? Correlation analysis was used to assess the relationship among the participants' gender, age, level of education, and the nonnative speakers' writing. Analysis of variance was used to assess the variation in the participants' writing associated with their native language. This study revealed that older students were significantly xnore likely to write longer compositions and to discuss more ideas than younger students. Average older students were also more involved with their essays than younger students. That is, older students tended to like the topic more, found it more interesting, and experienced less anxiety while writing. Writing 114 proficiency scores, text sophistication, global coherence, linguistic complexity, and revision strategies were not significantly related to the students' age. Level of education might be expected to play a significant role on the students' compositions; surprisingly, none of the writing measures except involvement was affected by the level of education. The study revealed that the higher the students' level of education, the more they were involved with their compositions. Involvement was also affected by how much students read. The amount of reading also could be expected to play a significant role on the quality and quantity of the students' compositions. Krashen (1984) and Stotsky (1983) said that ability to write was not taught, but rather was acquired through extensive reading, and therefore, better writers are necessarily better readers. This study suggested otherwise. Writing proficiency scores, content length, global coherence, linguistic complexity, and revision strategies, which appeared to represent writing ability, were not significantly related to the amount of reading the students reported that they did. Therefore, Krashen's notion of reading as the source of writing skill was not confirmed in this study. 115 Participants' Native Language This study disclosed that there were differences among the means of writing measures that can be associated with the subgroups‘ native language. For instance, the Arabic and Spanish subgroups outperformed the rest of the subgroups in the amount of subordination. Why or how elements of these subgroups' native language caused the difference in the subjects' writing performance is beyond the scope of this study. Prior Knowledge and Writing Performance 2. What is the relationship between prior knowledge about specific topics and the nonnative speakers' written discourse? The relationship between prior knowledge and each of the writing measures are more fully discussed below. Writing Tests This study expands the previous studies' findings to the ESL students' writing. The findings of this study revealed that great background knowledge of the topic about which they were writing affected the nonnative speakers' writing at the quantitative and qualitative levels. Specifically, the nonnative speakers with high prior knowledge scored higher in aa writing proficiency test similar to the writing holistic scores in the 116 previous studies of Langer and Chesky. On both scales, the students' compositions were scored holistically in terms of stating and supporting a position, passage relevance, organization, details, sentence structure, word choice, and mechanical errors. This finding was supported by Hilgers (1982) who also found that students familiar with content pay more attention to style, arrangement, grammar, and mechanics. Content Length The students with high knowledge were able to present and discuss many more ideas and to write longer essays than the students with low knowledge. In this study, there were some students with low knowledge who wrote relatively long essays, but their ideas were not relevant. They either discussed culturally related ideas or presented legends related to the phenomenon of Eclipses. Langer (1984), Chesky and Hiebert (1987), and Stroethoff (1988) contended that knowing much about the topic enabled the students to include and present more ideas, details, experiences, and concepts. This finding is also consistent with Perl's (1979) study which revealed that students write more information when they write about topics familiar to them, such as personal topics. 117 It is obvious that if writers know nothing about the topic, they will write very little. Some subjects in this study expressed their unwillingness to write on the topn: of eclipses because they said they did not know anything about it. A few of them agreed, but wrote only one sentence, "I know nothing about Eclipses," and handed in their papers at the end of the session. This polite refusal to participate appeared to reflect their agony at being asked to write (x1 a topic about which they knew nothing. Text Sophistication Text sophistication was measured by looking at the existence of elements, such as terminology, jargon, references, definitions, and explanations which presumably more characterized texts addressed to more sophisticated audiences. Concerning the relationship between prior knowledge and text sophistication, this study revealed different findings from those of Stroethoff (1988). In this study, the relationship between prior knowledge and text sophistication was apparent. In Stroethoff's study, the relationship between prior knowledge and text sophistication was not consistent. in the three topics on which. she asked her subjects to write. Two reasons may account for these different findings. One is that Stroethoff asked her 118 subjects to write on three topics and the variation of the subjects' knowledge about the three topics might be responsible for the inconsistency of the effect of prior knowledge on these two variables. Another reason is the difference in age and analytical ability between her subjects and the academically more sophisticated subjects of this study Inight account for the subjects of this study outperforming hers in these two variables. Langer (1984) and Chesky and Hiebert (1987) did not investigate their subjects' text sophistication. In text sophistication, the high knowledge subjects were able to use specialized terminology, jargon, and references which were only accessible to a highly educated and sophisticated audience. On the other hand, the low knowledge subjects tend to provide simple explanations of the notion of Eclipses. Interestingly, some high knowledge subjects drew some pictures of the sun and moon Eclipses and why and how they occur. Global Coherence This study revealed that prior knowledge was significantly correlated with global coherence. The texts of the students with high knowledge more clearly identified the topic, orient the readers, employ cohesive ties, plan and organize details, and followed the discourse structural rules. In contrast, the data 119 suggested that students with low knowledge tended to write fragmentary and haphazard texts. Those students tended not to sense where each idea fit, nor did they have a plan to organize or plan the details of their compositions. This study's finding was consistent with a similar study conducted by McCutchen (1986) who found that students with high knowledge about football were able to bring out and discuss important ideas of football more coherently than the students with low knowledge. This finding was also supported by Britton (1978) who asserted that high prior knowledge writers know which ideas are related and how all the parts are to be put together. Langer (1984) and Chesky and Hiebert (1987) found a positive significant relationship between prior knowledge and global coherence. Involvement Regarding involvement, this study revealed that high prior knowledge subjects were more involved in their compositions while writing them. The findings of this study suggest that it is important for writers to have a positive attitude toward the topic about which they are writing. This kind of attitude may create confidence, motivation, and lessen anxiety which will allow the flow of ideas and permit learning from writing to take place. Chesky (1984) indicates that high prior knowledge 120 subjects were more involved with their writing. As a result of their involvement, they wrote better and found the writing easier. Linguistic Complexity This study and all previous studies of this type did not reveal any relationship between prior knowledge and linguistic complexity. One suitable explanation of this finding was provided by Chesky (1984) who indicated that the mean T-unit length constitutes a problem as a method of measuring the quality of writing. The problem comes from the fact that T—unit length does not reflect the ability of students to carry out and construct meaning. Another problem with T—unit length as a measure of linguistic complexity is that it is not clear whether our subjects deliberately choose not to write long T- units, or were not able to write long T—units. This problem constitutes a major drawback of the mean of T- unit length as a measure of linguistic complexity. Furthermore, Witte and Faigley (1981) indicated the inappropriateness of using T—unit length as an indicator of writing quality. They contended that T-unit length and clause length have very little influence on the quality of writing. Although the T-unit measurement has received criticism, it is still the widely used 121 measurement for the syntactic complexity of the native and nonnative speaker's written discourse. Revision Strategies Revision strategies are an important stage in writing which helps student writers to construct clearly the intended message. The data of this study did not show a significant relationship between prior knowledge and revision strategies. One explanation might be that students spend most or all of the 45 minutes writing the compositions, not revising them. On the other hand, those who did not write long compositions may have spent their time thinking about new ideas, not revising them. This finding is consistent with Raimes' (1987) study which demonstrated that ESL student writers do not pay attention to their mistakes because they are language learners. Most of this study's subjects only pay attention to the writing down of ideas, leaving the mistakes to their teachers to correct. Prior Knowledge and Writipg Performance of the Subgroups when students are grouped by native language, the relationship among prior knowledge, writing proficiency scores, content length, text sophistication, global coherence, linguistic complexity, revision strategies, and involvement become much more complex. 122 For instance, the relationship between prior knowledge and writing proficiency scores was evident for the Arabic and Japanese subgroups only. Prior knowledge also consistently related to the length of the essays of all the subgroups. In the miscellaneous subgroups, text sophistication appeared to have a significant relationship with prior knowledge. No other subgroup's writing measures appeared to have a significant relationship with prior knowledge. This inconsistency of the relationship between prior knowledge and the subgroups' writing measures may be attributed to either or both of the following factors. One factor might be that the number in each subgroup was small. Another factor might have been an effect of the subgroups' native languages. Investigating this factor lies beyond the scope of this study. Overall Language Proficiency and Writing Performance 3. What is the relationship between overall language proficiency and the nonnative speakers' written discourse? This study revealed that there was a significant positive relationship between overall language proficiency and writing proficiency scores, text sophistication, and mean T-unit length for the whole group. No significant relationship was found between 123 overall language proficiency and length of the essays, global coherence, amount of subordination, revision strategies, and involvement for the whole group. For the homogeneous subgroups, the results of the relationships between overall language proficiency and writing measures were also very complex. For instance, for the Arabic subgroup, there was a significant positive relationship between overall language proficiency and revision strategies and also between overall language proficiency and text sophistication. For the .Japanese subgroups, there was also a significant positive relationship between overall language proficiency and mean T-unit length and amount of subordination. No other significant relationships were found between overall language proficiency and any of the writing Ineasures. Hence, it seems that placing students in writing classes according to their language proficiency scores alone may be a mistake. Additive Effect 4. What is the additive effect of prior knowledge and overall language proficiency on nonnative speakers' written discourse? The results of the study revealed that prior knowledge accounted for more of the variability in writing measures than overall language proficiency. 124 Specifically, prior knowledge accounted for more variability in writing proficiency scores, number of words, and number of ideas than overall language proficiency did. The overall language proficiency, in contrast, accounted for more variability on only mean T- unit length than prior knowledge did. Implication The findings of this study demonstrated that familiarity with the topic affected writing proficiency scores, content length, text sophistication, coherence, and involvement with the text. Based on the findings of this study, writing instructors might consider the following: 1. Choose interesting topics and ask students to read and discuss materials related to those 'topics in prewriting activities. Instructors should ask their students to write about topics that are interesting to them. Once the instructors make sure that the topic interests them, they then ask the students to read some materials related to that topic in order to activate. either their existing schemata about that topic or to provide them with information that would enable them to write well. Another alternative is dividing the classroom into groups and asking them to discuss collaboratively the topic. In A 125 this way students could exchange information about the topic students are writing. 2. Prior knowledge should take its place with language proficiency in the writing class This study revealed that prior knowledge was more strongly related to students' writing than language proficiency was. Therefore, in writing classes, language should not be overemphasized since it does not account for the majority of the variability in the students' writing performance. 3. Necessary jargon and vocabulary must be provided to students During the administration of the writing assignment, students appeared to have a lot of questions regarding the topic about which they were to write. Some questions related to clarifying facts, others related to the vocabulary needed to express specialized facts. Therefore, if the topic requires explanation of abstract facts or requires jargon, the vocabulary should be provided. 4. Ideas relevant to the issues being discussed in the topic must be highlighted in prewriting activities This study revealed that one problem of ESL student writers lay in recalling ideas irrelevant to the issue they were discussing. One way to solve this :- 126 problem is by utilizing the prewriting session to highlight the relevant ideas of the topic. Doing so will help the students to learn which ideas fit what issue, and will, in turn, help them generate well organized and coherent texts. Carrell (1983) said that ESL students do not utilize prior knowledge properly when they read. This finding seems to apply to some writers since some of their essays contained interesting cultural ideas about eclipses, but these ideas did not fit or support the issues they were discussing. Consequently, it is important for the students to learn how to separate scientific facts from cultural legends. For example, if ideas are unrelated to the issues being discussed, they should not be included in the essay. 5. The students' native language could be initially used to generate ideas Lee (1986) and Swaffar (1988) indicated that native language has an advantage over a second language in the process of recalling information about the text to be read. Therefore, students might be asked to jot down ideas in their first language and then, with the assistance of their instructors translate what they have written into the second language. 6. Grades should not be emphasized as the ultimate goal of writing 127 Based on observation during the conduct of this study, we believe that grades should not be emphasized because students usually fear grading. Therefore, students should be told that they are not writing to attain grades. The agony of writing for grades, as Chesky (1984) pointed out, may keep the students from freely expressing their thoughts and from trying different styles. Of course, grades encourage and motivate students, especially the good students, but grades may also deter others. 7. The prewriting activities should not be understated or overstated Prewriting activities must be wisely planned and organized to avoid wasting time and effort. Spending too little or too much time preparing the students to write may aggravate more than help the preparation process. This matter is actually a judgmental one. Suggestions for Further Research For further research the following suggestions could be considered: 1. Academic progress, study skills, and intelligence should be studied along with prior knowledge and language proficiency We have learned while conducting this study that there could be a third variable in addition to content 128 and language proficiency. Perhaps the good writers were the ones who had good study skills and who happens to know more about the topic. In addition, they might also be the kind of students who would be disciplined enough to be good writers. So, it may be that when this study measures skills, it not only measured writing and background knowledge, but it measured something else that really governed both of them. Variables, such as academic progress, study skills, academic sophistication, and intelligence, may not only cause students to write well, but may also allow them to be more knowledgeable. So, it may be that it is not the students' prior knowledge that affects their writing, but rather, something else such as the above—mentioned variables. Consequently, we suggest that any subsequent study take into account the effect of those variables on the students' writing performance. 2. students' native language should be used for testing the students' prior knowledge As indicated earlier, the students' native language may provide an advantage over their second language in expressing the knowledge they have about the assigned topic. Therefore, we suggest that subsequent studies ask students to express their knowledge in their first language. In this way, a full picture of students' knowledge about the topic will be obtained. 129 3. students' linguistic levels should be measured in advance In this study, we do not know whether the linguistic complexity reflected in the students' compositions was a result of the students' knowledge about the topic or a production of their actual linguistic level. To control this variable, the students' level of linguistic complexity should be tested beforehand. As a result of this testing, a group of students whose proficiency is the same should be chosen as a sample for any subsequent similar studies. 4. To measure revision strategies, a designated time for revisions should be allowed Students could be given an allotted time for revisions. Most of the students expended the given time to generate meaning, but not to clean up their compositions. We speculate that the lack of relationship between prior knowledge and revision strategies was due to the lack of using time to revise. Whether this is true or not would be proven by asking the same subjects to write a second draft which focuses on clarifying the meaning they have generated in the first draft. 130 Summary This chapter has presented a summary of the conclusions of this study, a discussion of the findings, implications of the findings, and suggestions for further studies. Prior knowledge apparently seems to be related to writing proficiency scores, content length, text sophistication, global coherence, and involvement. T— unit length, amount of subordination, and revision strategies appear to be unrelated to prior knowledge. Overall, language proficiency seems to have a relationship with writing proficiency scores, text sophistication, and linguistic complexity. Content length, global coherence, revision strategies, and involvement were not related to overall language proficiency. This study is consistent with similar studies in finding that prior knowledge is crucial to writing. Accordingly, we suggest that writing instructors ask their students to write about topics interesting to them and to de-emphasize the teaching of language in writing classes. Further, we suggest that instructors provide jargon and specialized terminology, highlight the issues of the topic, encourage students not to discuss peripheral ideas, and not overstate testing for preparing their students for writing assignments. 131 For subsequent research, we suggest the following: first, research should take into account the students' academic progress, study skills, and academic sophistication. Second, they should use the students' native language in testing their prior knowledge. Third, testing the students' linguistic complexity in advance. And finally, since revision strategies cannot be judged from the first draft, students should be asked to write a second draft focusing on revision strategies. APPENDICES APPENDIX A MISCELLANEOUS CORRESPONDENCE :_~”, —. April 6, 1988 Dr. James Stalker English Language Center Michigan State University East Lansing, MI 48823 Dear Dr. Stalker: I am a Ph.D. student in the English Department of Michigan State University, and in the process of conducting research for my dissertation on the effect of nonnative speakers' prior knowledge on their expository writing. This letter is to request permission to use the English Language Center as a setting for the research. Students :u1 D- and E-levels will be asked to write an essay during one of their regular class periods. They will also be asked to respond to a questionnaire, take tests, and demonstrate their prior knowledge of three topics during two extra special sessions at the ELC. Taking into account the students' time and effort, an arrangement with their teachers will be sought to make the students' participation consistent with the teachers' endeavors to improve the students' writing. As a result of the students' taking part in this study, they will benefit by getting four extra test scores (which they value), and doing the writing assignment. The study will also contribute to the field of teaching writing, to TESOL, and the program at ELC. I would also like to request sixty copies of the ELC grammar, vocabulary, and reading sample tests for the purpose of measuring the students' language proficiency. I appreciate your kind attention to this matter. Sincerely yours, Ali S. K. Al—Ghamdi MICHIGAN STATE UNIVERSITY ENGLISH LANGUAGE CENTER EAST LKNSING ' MICHIGAN ' 48824-1055 CENTER FOR INTERNATIONAL PROGRAMS April 11, 1988 Mr. Ali 5. K. Al-Ghamdi 2309 E. Jolly Road Lansing, MI 48910 Dear Mr. Al-Ghamdi: Your request to conduct research in the D and E Writing classes at the English Language Center is hereby granted. I think your research will be of use to our instructional staff in our continuing curricular considerations, and your research should improve the quality of instruction for future students. You may begin your research as soon as we have a copy of your proposal and the permission of the University Committee on Research on Human Subjects on file. Sincerely yours, 10,...ch James C. Stalker Director JCS:rhc cc: Dr. Barry Gross, Department of English .WSL' u an Affirmative Action/Equal Oppnrlumty IVISHIIIHOH 136 April 11, 1988 MEMORANDUM TO: D and E Writing Teachers: Boyd, Lazarowicz, Leonhardt, Milton, Novak, Wolf FROM: James C. Stalker, Director RE: Ali Al-Ghamdi's Research Ali Al-Ghamdi, who is completing his doctorate in the English Department, has my permission to conduct research in the writing classes at the ELC. He is currently awaiting official approval of the University Committee on Research on Human Subjects. As soon as he receives that, he will be able to move forward. He will be coming to you to explain his research and tell you what help he will need, but essentially he will want to hand some material out in our class and have our students do some writing for him during one of your class periods. He will, of course, schedule his needs around your requirements. If you have any questions, please see me. JCS:rhc cc: Mr. Ali—Al-Ghamdi 137 MICHIGAN STATE UNIVERSITY DEPARTMENT OF ENGLISH EAST LANSING O MICHIGAN ' 488244056 MORRILI. HALL April 6, 1988 John Hudzik, Chair UCRIHS 206 Berkey Dear Dr. Hudzik: Attached is the documentation requested and a full copy of Ali S.K. Al-Ghamdi's research proposal which has been approved by his Guidance Committee. We request exemption from full committee review on the basis of 1C, page 133 of UCRIHS guidelines. We appreciate your kind attention to this matter as soon as possible. Siesrely yours, /,,-’> . Paul E. Munsell Associate Professor PElefh Enclosure MIC-'1 an Affirmative Aztlan/Equal Opportunuy Inmluunn ”:94 c... MICHIGAN STATE UNIVERSITY UNIVERSITY COMMITTEE ON RESEARCH INVOLVING EAST LANSING ' MICHIGAN 0 48824-1111 HUMAN SUBJECTS (UCRIHS) 206 BERKEY HALL 1517) 553-9738 April 20, 1988 Ali S.K. Al—Ghamdi Dept. of English Morrill Hall Dear Mr. Al—Ghamdi: Subject: "THE EFFECT OF PRIOR KNOWLEDTGE ON NON-NATIVE SPEAKERS EXPOSITORY WRITING #88-120" The above project is exempt from full UCRIHS review. I have reviewed this project and approval is granted for conduct of this project. You are reminded that UCRIHS approval is valid for one calendar year. If you plan to continue this project beyond one year, please make provisions for obtaining appropriate UCRIHS approval prior to April 20, 1989. Any changes in procedures involving human subjects must be reviewed by the UCRIHS prior to initiation of the change. UCRIHS must also be notified promptly of any problems (unexpected side effects, complaints, etc.) involving human subjects during the course of the work. Thank you for bringing this project to our attention. If we can be of any future help, please do not hesitate to let us know. ohn K. Hudzik, Ph.D. hair, UCRIHS JKH/sar cc: P. Munsell "5L 1: .m :V/Irmum-e .dl'rtlm/Equul ()ppnrmmlv Insutumm .1“ 139 Dear Participant: I am a Ph.D. student in the English Department at Michigan State University. I am conducting research for my dissertation on nonnative writing. Your participation is highly important to the completion of the study, and will also assist you in improving your writing. You will write one essay and respond to a questionnaire and to three short probes of your knowledge. In addition, and as ea benefit to you, you will also take three English Language Center (ELC) type tests and get the results. You will also get an ELC—type score for your essays. Your responses will be completely confidential; please do not write your name anywhere on your questionnaire, essays, or tests. Participation in the study is voluntary; without penalty, you may choose not to answer certain questions, or not to participate at all. Thank you very much for your time and effort in helping me complete this study. Sincerely, Ali S. K. Al—Ghamdi Ph.D. Candidate APPENDIX B BACKGROUND INFORMATION QUESTIONNAIRE 141 Rater No. l ( ) Rater No. 2 ( ) Rater No. 3 ( ) Student : I - Demographic Items: 1 — Your Age: ( ) less than 20 years ( ) 20— 24 years ( ) 25— 29 years ( ) 30—34 years ( ) 35—39 years ( ) 40—44 years ( ) 45—49 years ( ) 50 years or above 2 — Please indicate your level of education you have completed: ( ) High school ( ) College ( ) Master's degree ( ) Other (please specify ) 3 - Gender: ( ) Male ( ) Female 4 — As well as I recall, my average in the last ELC tests was 5 - My native language is 6 — How much reading do you do? I do very much reading I do much reading I do little reading I do no reading at all AAAA VVVV APPENDIX C WORD ASSOCIATION/PRIOR KNOWLEDGE TEST 143 II - PRIOR KNOWLEDGE: TOPIC NO. 1, Photography: The following words/phrases represent the central ideas of the topic, photography Please write what you know about these words/ phrases. The examples below illustrate how you should respond to these words/phrases.. (Remember that you will not be identified nor will you receive any grade for doing this exercise.) Example 1, Trees: Prompt No. 1, Evergreen trees: Associations for the phrase "evergreen trees": -— The trees that do not lose their leaves in fall, but remain green all winter. -- The trees with slender and sharp-pointed leaves. -- The trees with leaves that stay on the trees for several years before dying -- Trees that do not change colors. -- Trees with short branches at the top, and long branches at the bottom. -- Come from cold weather places. -- Protect themselves from freezing weather and winter storms. Examples 2, Weather: Prompt No. 1, Summer season: I will write the phrase "Summer season” on the board, and we will practice together writing associations. Think about what you know about "Summer season" and raise your hand if you have something to say. This exerc1se 15 to make sure that you understand how you should write associations. 144 Now, start writing associations to the following prompts of the topic Photography. Prompt No. 1, Camera: Prompt No. 3, Negatives: Prompt NO. 4, Focusing: 145 146 Prompt No. 5, Flash: 147 Topic No. 2, Underwater Diving: The following words/phrases represent the central ideas of the topic, underwater diving: Prompt NO. 1, Skin diving: Prompt No. 2, Scuba Diving" 148 Prompt No. 3, Underwater Swimming: Prompt No. 4, Diving Equipment: 149 Prompt No. 5, Oceanography: 150 Topic No. 3, Eclipses: The following words/phrases represent the central idea of the topic, Eclipses. Prompt No. 2, Shadow of Eclipses: Prompt No. 2, Solar Eclipses: Prompt No. 3, Lunar Eclipses: Prompt No. 4, Astronomers: 151 152 Prompt No. 5, Eclipses Prediction: APPENDIX D SCORING THE PROMPTS 154 Rater #: Student: III - Scoring the prompts: A - Fluency: ( ) high fluency ( ) moderate fluency ( ) low fluency B — Organization: Write scores for all the prompts: ( ) ( ) ( ) ( ) ( ) Total = Average = ( ) High Organized ( ) Partially organized ( ) Diffusely organized C — Combination: Average of the scores of high organized and partially organized ( ) Highly combined ( ) Moderately combined ( ) Low Topic No. 1 155 Rater #: . Topic No. Student: . III - Scoring the prompts: A — Fluency: ( ) High fluency ( ) Moderate fluency ( ) Low fluency B - Organization: Write scores for all the prompts: ( ) ( ) ( ) ( ) ( ) Total Average ( ) Highly organized ( ) Partially organized ( ) Diffusely organized C - Average of the scores of high organized and partially organized ( ) Highly combined ( ) Moderately combined ( ) Low 156 Rater #: . Topic No. Student: . III - Scoring the prompts: A — Fluency: ( ) High fluency ( ) Moderate fluency ( ) Low fluency B - Organization: Write scores for all the prompts: ( > ( ) ( ) ( ) ( ) Total = Average = ( ) Highly organized ( ) Partially organized ( ) Diffusely organized C — Average of the scores of high organized and partially organized ( ) Highly combined ( ) Moderately combined ( ) Low 3 APPENDIX E THE WRITING TASK AND THE INVOLVEMENT QUESTIONNAIRE 158 Rater No. 1 ( ) Rater No. 2 ( ) Rater No. 3 ( ) IV - Writing the essay: You have forty-five minutes to write about the topic: Eclipses You should assume that you will be writing this essay for well-educated readers. You can write as much as you can. (Please use pens for writing.) 159 Involvement: Please put a ( ) beside the appropriate answer as it applies to you: Liking the topic: ( ( ( ( ) Level Vvv AAA“ Level I like this topic very much I like this topic I don't have a firm opinion about liking or disliking it I don't like it. of difficulty as writing about this topic: It It It It is very easy to write about is easy to write about this is hard to write about this is very hard to write about this topic topic topic this topic of interest and excitement while writing on this topic: was highly interested was interested was somewhat interested was not interested HHHH of anxiety: not nervous while writing on this topic somewhat nervous while writing on this I was I was topic I was I was topic nervous while writing on this topic very nervous while writing on this of confidence: I felt very confident while writing on this topic . . I felt confident while writing on this topic I felt somewhat confident while writing on this topic . . I didn't feel very confident while writing on this topic 6 160 — If there is a chance I will write about this topic again: strongly agree agree somewhat agree don't agree HHHH ( ( ( ( — I have enjoyed writing about this topic: strongly agree agree somewhat agree ( E ( don't agree HHHH - Who did you have in mind as your reader when you were writing? ( ) An expert reader in this topic ( ) An average reader with only some knowledge ( ) A reader with little knowledge ( ) A reader with no knowledge APPENDIX F SCORING THE PARTICIPANTS' WRITTEN DISCOURSE TEXT SOPHISTICATION QUESTIONNAIRE Rater # Student VI — Level of English proficiency: ELC Score VII — Linguistic Complexity: A — T—Unit length Mean of number of words per t—unit: Number of words in the essay = Number of T-units in the essay = B — Subordination Ratio of main clauses to all clauses Number of all the clauses: Number of main clauses: VIII - Writing global language proficiency score: Score: IX — Global coherence: Miscellaneous Incomprehensible Incoherent Partially coherent Fully coherent XI XII 163 - Revision Strategies: — Internal revision ( ) Number of marks in subject ( ) Number of marks in information and arguments ( ) Number of marks in structure - External revision: ( ) Number of marks in tone and style ( ) Number of marks in language ( ) Number of marks in mechanics - ( ) Total revision - Content: - Number of words — Fluency (central ideas) ( ) High fluency ( ) Moderate fluency ( ) Low fluency — Text sophistication: - The complexity of terminology and/or jargon of essay: Highly complex Complex Less complex Not complex Vvvv The level of sophistication of references: ( ) Very sophisticated ( ) Mature but simple ( ) Very general ( ) Child like (very simple) Definitions and Explanation: Not found at all Very few Few Very many AAA“ VVVV 4 8 164 - Repetitions: ( ) No repetitions ( ) Very few repetitions ( ) A few repetitions ( ) A lot of repetitions - Is there any mentioning of the audience to which they are addressing their writing? Examples: - Is there any indications that show their awareness of the audience? Examples: - False start Yes . No - Stray thoughts Yes . No Examples APPENDIX G PROCEDURES FOR SELECTING PROMPTS PROCEDURES OF SCORING KNOWLEDGE ORGANIZATION ENGLISH LANGUAGE CENTER WRITING SCORING SCALE BAMBERG COHERENCE SCALE 166 PROCEDURES FOR SELECTING PROMPTS (From Newell & McAdam, 1987) Care should be taken not to select words/phrases that are semantically close, such as antonyms (hot—cold) and synonyms (large—enormous). When words/phrases in the content structure are accompanied by modifiers (for example, large, white automobile) that do not change the meaning of the concept, the modifiers are excluded. However, if the modifier(s) does affect the meaning of the word/ phrase (for example, welfare program), the modifier is included. Proper nouns are not selected unless they are critical to understanding the passage. For example, in a passage about Stonehenge, Stonehenge would be selected but not England. When the passage includes both a technical term (deficit spending) and its synonym (spending money one does not have), the technical term should be selected. 167 PROCEDURES OF SCORING KNOWLEDGE ORGANIZATION (From Newell & McAdam, 1987) Each association is rated separately and independently at only one level of knowledge organization. Responses are rated according to how the concepts are used 1J1 the selected passages (definitions for each tenn are available to raters). For example, if the concept "elements" was selected from a passage on the chemistry of compounds and mixtures, responses related to "weather" would have to be rated as diffusely organized knowledge. Incomplete definitions and imprecise word use may be "defining characteristics" (partially organized) rather than definitions (well organized). For example, "elements" defined as "things that make up other things" indicates 3partially organized knowledge. Attention to level of specificy is important in rating a response. A response may appear to be an example, attribute, or defining characteristic of a concept but too broad or general and therefore peripherally linked only to the concept (for example, the response "money" for the prompt "financial rewards"). When the prompt or its root is used as part of the response with little or no elaboration (for example, "Neutrality: to remain neutral"), such a response is rated "diffusely organized" unless there is further elaboration in the response indicating a higher level of organization. 168 When the prompt consists of two or more words (for example, "Internal Affairs: matters concerned with the inner workings of government such as collection of taxes"), the response should be rated as representative of a high level of organization only if it indicates understanding of the prompt as a concept; responses that define only one of the words, such as "affairs inside a country," would be rated as partially organized. In cases where the students have not indicated any separation between their string of responses, raters should use the following guidelines for determining what qualifies as a single response: (a) phrases "join the army," "fight for the country," (b) clauses "because of the need for food," (c) sentences that are clearly marked by a period, (d) single words in a string of responses. 100 95 9O 85 8O 75 169 ENGLISH LANGUAGE CENTER SCORING SCALE Very clear. Native grammatical and vocabulary usage. Few if any misspelled words or punctuation problems. About 2 pages or more. Between 90 and 100. Very clear. Few grammatical errors of any kind. Generally smooth. Good command of vocabulary with not more than several inaccurate or unnatural usages. May have a few misspellings or punctuation problems. Well organized. Clearly developed paragraphs. Between 80 and 90. Apparent that writer is not a native speaker. Good organization and paragraph development. Typical of a very advanced ELC student. Very clear and understandable with minor obscure parts. Generally accurate use of grammatical structures at all levels with some problems with advanced structures. Mostly smooth sentences with a few choppy or run-on constructions. Largely accurate use of vocabulary with some problems with advanced words and some unnatural usage. A few misspelled or distorted words, and a few punctuation problems. Adequate attention to organization and development. Clear with possibly one or two parts that require some mental editing. Generally accurate on elementary and intermediate grammatical structures, with occasional serious errors on advanced structures. Generally smooth, with a few choppy or poorly connected or run—on clauses or sentences. Some use of advanced vocabulary with several incorrectly or unnaturally used words. Several misspelled or distorted words and several punctuation problems. Some attention to overall organization and development. About 1—1/2 pages long. 7O 65 6O 55 45 170 Clear with minor editing but may have some parts requiring major editing or guessing. Frequent minor grammatical errors, but also accurate or nearly accurate use of a wide range of structures and tenses. Often smooth but with several poorly connected or choppy or run—on clauses or sentences. Often smooth but with several poorly connected or choppy or run-on clauses or sentences. Vocabulary largely elementary, but often advanced words are used, although. not always accurately. Possibly many misspelled words and up to several distortions. Sentences usually clearly marked but with several punctuation problems. Probably some unclear parts. Use of a wide range of grammatical structures but with numerous errors, minor and serious, especially on the more advanced structures. Several choppy, run-on, or poorly connected clauses or sentences. Vocabulary range in the elementary and intermediate ranges but with frequent errors at all levels, especially with advanced words . Quite a few unnatural usages . Probably many misspelled words or distortions. Punctuation may show frequent problems. Usually I to 1—1/2 pages. Probably becomes unclear or confusing when expressing some complex ideas. Sometimes accurate at the elementary level but with serious inaccuracies at all levels. Many choppy, run-on or poorly connected clauses or sentences. Many serious errors at all levels, in vocabulary, though fair use of elementary vocabulary. Frequent unnatural or apparently translated or invented words. Probably many misspellings, distortions, or mispunctuations. About a page or more. Probably be unclear in several parts or may possibly attempt to express only very elementary ideas. Few accurate sentences or natural and adequate use of vocabulary. Rarely smooth. Many apparent translations or invented words. Many misspellings, distortions, and nuspunctuations. About one page. May be largely unclear or overly simple. Few accurate or complex sentences. Inaccurate use of vocabulary, even at elementary level. Usually less than one page. 171 35 Name plus attempts at one or two sentences. Barely recognizable as English. 30 Name only; no attempt to write. 172 HOLISTIC COHERENCE SCALE 4 = Fully Coherent Writer clearly identifies the topic Writer does not shift topics or digress Writer orients the reader by creating a context or situation Writer organizes details according to a discernible plan that is sustained throughout the essay Writer skillfully 'uses cohesive ties such. as lexical cohesion, conjunction, reference, etc. to link sentences and/or paragraphs together Writer often concludes with a statement that gives the reader a definite sense of closure Discourse flows smoothly-~few or no grammatical and/or mechanical errors interrupt the reading process 3 = Partially Coherent If writer does not explicitly identify the topic, s/he provides enough details so that readers can probably identify the specific subject Writer has one main topic, but there may be minor digressions Writer provides some reader orientation, either by briefly suggesting the context or by directly announcing the topic Writer organizes details according to a plan, but may not sustain it throughout or may list details in part of the essay Writer uses some cohesive ties such as lexical cohesion, conjunction, reference, etc., to link sentences and/or paragraphs together 173 Writer does not usually conclude with a statement that creates a sense of closure Discourse generally flows smoothly although. occasional grammatical and/or mechanical errors may interrupt the reading process 2 = Incoherent Some of the following prevent the reader from integrating the text into a coherent whole: Writer does not identify the topic and the reader would be unlikely to infer or guess the topic from the details provided Writer shifts topics or «digresses frequently from 'the topic Writer assumes the reader shares his/her context and provides little or no orientation Writer has no organizational plan in most of the text and frequently relies on listing Writer uses few cohesive ties such as lexical cohesion, conjunction, reference, etc. to link sentences and/or paragraphs together Writer creates no sense of closure Discourse flow is irregular or rough because mechanical and/or grammatical errors frequently interrupt the reading process 1 = Incomprehensible Many of 'the following' prevent the reader from Inaking sense of the text: Topic cannot be identified Writer move from topic to topic by association or digresses frequently Writer assumes the reader shares his/her context and provides no orientation 174 Writer has no organizational plan and either lists or follows an associative order Writer uses very few cohesive ties such as lexical cohesion, conjunction, reference, etc. and sentences do not seem connected or linked together Discourse flow is very rough or irregular because writer omits structure words, inflectional endings and/or makes numerous grammatical and :mechanical errors that continuously interrupt the reading process 0 = Unscorable/Miscellaneous Source: Bamberg (1984). APPENDIX H THE STUDY'S RAW DATA 13:? 20—24 25-29 30—34 35—40 40 or UlnthH II II II II II L.E. (Level of IbLAJNF‘ G.R. (Gender) Male 176 years years years years over Education) M.A. degree B.A. degree High school degree Others Female ELC Score (Language Proficiency Scores (35—100)) R (Amount of Reading) 1 2 AFL (Fluency) 1 LOW 2 3 High Little reading Much reading Moderate AOG Organization (Scale 5—15) ACM Combination (Scale 0-15) LCP (T—Unit Length) Sub (Subordination) WLP (WritingiGlobal Language Proficiency) WRD (Number of Words) IDS (Number of Ideas Scale: 1—3) 177 TNT 4(Total of T-Units) GC (Global Coherence Scale: 1—4) COH (Cohesion Scale: 1-4) TS (Text Sophistication) AA (Audience Awareness Scale: 1—4) INV (Involvement) ER (External Revision) IR (Internal Revision) TR (Total Revision) 178 Table H—l eonm 11H.1163m mncm<.m not cons Idln zmnH2. 500 so: How moo zoo 2.5 How 5.25. on no: em >5 :2 m» Ho .3 N N H NN N N N.NN N.NN N.NN N.00 mm Hmb H H0 N.NN N.NN .5365 N N H No N N Ho. NN N.NN H0.NN N .3 NN NNN N NN N.oo N .00 W w H HM w HM N N H N0 N N HN.NN HN.NN N.00 H.m0 NN NN5 N NN N.NN N.00 H5 N Hm HN H Hm N N H Nm N N HH.NN H0.NN H0.00 N.05 N5 NNO N NN 5.00 5.00 H0 N Hm HN 0 H N N H N0 H N HN.00 HN.NN HN.NN H.00 NN N00 N Na N.NN N.00 Hm N H0 H5 0 NW N N H NH H N H5.00 H5.00 H0.NN H.mN N5 N00 N Nm N.00 N.00 H5 5 H5 H0 0 N0 orHsmmm N N N No H N N.NN N.NN H0.0H H.m0 00 N50 N NN 5.00 5.00 Hm N HN H5 N HN N N N 2. N N 8.8 N.NN HH.NN HNN NN 58 N No 5.00 N.NN HN N Hm No H NH m N H NN N N HN.00 HN.NN HN.05 H.0H N0 NNo N No 5.00 5.00 HN N HN N H N 5 N H mm N N HH.NN H0.NN HH.5H H.5m NN Nmm N NN 5.00 5.00 HN N NN 0 N HH N N N NN H N Poo N.NN HH.No H.No No NNN N No N.NN N.NN HH N Ho H o H 5 N H No N N 0.00 0.00 H0.00 H.Nm Nm Nmo N Nm N.NN N.NN HN N Hm H 0 H N N N 00 N N H0.00 N.NN HN.00 N.NN N5 NNH N NN N.00 N.00 HH H N H0 H N0 5 N N 0N H H N.00 0.00 N.NN H.Nm Nm Nmm N No 5.00 N.NN HH N H0 HN H HN 5 N H N0 H H N.NN N.NN N.NN H.0m NN HNm N N0 N.NN N.NN Hm N N m N m omomommo N N H N0 H H HH.NN H0.NN H0.NN H.5m Nm NNN N NN N.NN N.NN HH N HN HN o HN N N H NN H N 0.00 0.00 H0.NN H.N5 N5 50N N NN N.00 N.00 N H HH N5 0 N5 N H N 0N N N H0.NN H0.NN HH.H0 N.0N NN NNN N N0 N.00 5.00 H0 H N m m H0 N N H NN H N HH.NN HH.00 H0.N0 H.5H 00 NmN N N5 5.00 5.00 HN N Ho m N m 5 N N 00 N N HN.NN HH.NN HN.HN H.mN 00 Now N N0 5.00 5.00 HN N H0 Nm 0 Nm N N N N0 N H N.NN HN.NN N.NN H.Nm mm HoN H HN N.00 H.mN HH N HN m 0 m N H H NN H N N.NN N.NN 0.05 H.5N NN NNm N Nm 5.00 N.NN HN N m HN 0 HN m N H 00 N N H0.NN N.NN HH.00 H.mN mH Nmm N N5 5.00 5.00 Hm N Hm NN 0 NH H H N NN H H N.NN 0.00 0.00 H.Nm NN 00 H H0 N.00 N.00 HH N H0 N 0 N N H H NN H N N.NN N.NN HN.00 H.5N NN NH5 N No 5.00 5.00 HN N H0 H0 N H0 5 N H No H H H0.NN N.NN H0.5m H.Nm NN NmN N Nm N.00 N.00 HN N 0 N0 m N5 N N N Na H H N.NN N.NN H5.mm H.N0 mm H00 H HH N.00 H.0N H0 N 0 5m H 50 5 N N 0N N N HN.NN HN.00 H0.00 H.N0 00 50m N Na N.NN N.NN HN 5 H0 Hm N Hm N N H mm H N HH.NN H0.NN HH.NN H.N0 Nm NNN N Nm N.00 N.00 H5 5 HH NH N N5 N N N 00 H N H0.NN H0.00 HH.NN H.m5 N0 N50 N N0 N.NN 5.00 H0 H N H0 0 H0 N 5 H mN N N HN.NN HN.NN N.NN H.NN N5 NmH N No N.00 N.00 HH N Ho HH 0 HH N H N 0N N N 0.00 N.00 HN.NN N.00 NN Nmm N NH N.00 N.NN HH N HH 0 N HH N H H N0 H N HH.00 H0.NN N.NN H.mN Nm H50 N HN N.NN N.00 HN H m H H N N H N NH H N H0.00 N.NN 0.H0 H.NN NH NH H H0 N.00 N.00 0 H N N 5 N N H H N0 N N HH.NN H0.NN N.NN H.Nm N0 N05 N NN 5.00 5.00 H5 N H5 Hm N HN N H N N0 H H 0.00 0.00 HH.NN H.mm Nm NN5 N H0 N.00 N.NN Hm N H0 H5 N Hm N N H N0 H H N.NN N.00 N.Nm H.5m Nm NOH N N0 5.00 N.NN H0 H m NN m NN N N H 00 H N N.NN N.NN N.NN H.5H Na NHH N 50 5.00 5.00 H0 H N 5m m m0 H H N N5 H N 0.00 N.00 N.NN H.wm 0N 505 H NN N.NN N.00 HH N H0 0 0 0 momon: m N N mm H N HN.00 HN.NN HH.H5 H.NN NN NHN N NN N.00 N.00 HN N Hm m 0 m H H 5 N0 H N H5.00 H5.00 Hm.5m N.NN N5 HNo N HH N.00 N.NN HN H H0 HN 0 HN H H H 00 H N HH.00 0.00 HN.No H.mN 00 NN5 N N5 N.NN N.mN HN N HN HN H HN HancommHo: H H N 00 H N HN.00 N.00 H0.0N H.NN NN 5NN N 5N 5.00 5.00 H5 N HN NN 5 No m N N 00 N N HN.00 HN.00 HN.50 H.N5 N0 N50 N Nm 5.00 N.NN HN H Hm 0 0 0 w N H N0 N H H0.00 N.NN 0.00 H.mm NN HNm N Hm N.00 N.NN HN N HH N m 0 a N H NN N N HH.NN HH.NN H0.NN H 0H N5 NHH N No N.00 N.00 HN N N0 H5 N Ho 5 N N. mNI, M 111:-rIINHQQIIIJHHHHIIIHHWHHIiz:NWQQI--Nflsll as H N chc1 u.cc HMI H1. N m H m N N N NN N H N.NN 5.NN H0.NN H.Na N5 55m H 5N N.00 N.NN H0 H H5 N N m N N H NH H N H0.NN N.NN N.NN H.N0 Nm :3 N H0 N.NN N.NN HN H HH H o H APPENDIX I PRIOR KNOWLEDGE TEST AND WRITING SAMPLES 180 STUDENT 1 Personal Characteristics Age: 40-44 Level of Education: M.A. Gender: Male Amount of Reading: Much reading Prior Knowledge Test Fluency: High Organization: High Combined: High Writing Performance Overall Language Proficiency Score: 73 Holistic Writing Score: 71 Number of Words: 311 Number of Ideas: 3 Text Sophistication: Moderate Global Cohererce: Partially coherent Involvement: Highly involved Total Number of T—units: 30 Mean T—unit Length: 10.37 Amount of Subordination: Partial Total Number of Revisions: 16 students' Associations to the Prompts Topic No. 3: Eclipses: The following words/phrases represent the central ideas of the topic, Eclipses. Prompt No. 1, Shadow of Eclipses: --Comes to the earth —-Will dark in the earth where the shadow comes --Some time make someone's afraid —-The plant will not process its activities -—The place will get dark a moment -—The place where the shadow comes like at night 181 Prompt No. 2, Solar Eclipses: --The sun doesn't look clearly --There is a total eclipses and part eclipses —-There is a story about this eclipses among the people --The story about the giant eat the sun -—The earth will get dark a moment '--Happen in the day --There is an effect to the tea --Many people to watch it by glasses Prompt No. 3, Lunar Eclipses: --The people will not see the moon clearly --Happen in the night --The people get unhappy because they are afraid —-The moon looks a part or total --Happens in the full moon --There is an effect to the sea Prompt No. 4, Astronomers: --The persons who expect in the astronomy —-They make account to the star --They work with instruments --They are graduate from the engineering faculty Prompt No. 5, Eclipses Prediction: --Made by astronomers ——The dark can be predicted --Sometime a year happen twice or once --Be counted by detail numerical point -—Can't be found by commoner 182 2.4554 awe...“ é/eumz? ‘M- @MWAN/ M7 4% M/oawém WWW“ 1;; ”Mae. fl» (M 444/? £954, 74%«2/‘4/5‘12 «xv-4H; cod/ea WK 49mm Mac/Ha ,Waz Hoes/Ha W @114; 517/47»; A244. 0.4» «565“ W M/f% ”Hz/HQ " Jaw ALA ’4 Hag/52,637. wad 75465., %%mA/N (AZ. Has/NH Nam/H ma. /% 45mm. J‘Léz/érmfl N.NN/{NM ”'46; M,M.;r MM... M/Aé72/flr 45.... 7%xwrm),éfiv%e m £9,262.34 [Hex/64; «ya/m» 74/4 4mm. W ,4. $455.. afl¢4aam WWW f 442;”... H£Hw M fl¢M, 72;; dokxaz/éé é/fiw m%cz/W%m a 1744/. éM/éac. %.W Mflwém /WW 4.; 44-.-MHH7, WM- W _ 4...“, 4i flLHf/WWH W2 £744: 7W ”(é/14; oa/m Zd%.ao’e‘ W W ”MW/7&W 4'72 71/7,: mm . M35. .242... ywwamwfl HN.NN/4...... H456, 4.;me HA7, W44 MPH/A7 aél/im Hag/He c2». 4;. - flNnaeg/zxa ng ”H, 4726 M,/;,452 flM,/&a¢¢a€ 183 , é/N- flZWé W ifxfo/ 4% v‘zm M .72.:- m 4. A? A 4274 W, rnu{ gflésy / 1,4/ 4% flamevn744 £5,752 (Ha/55¢ ”dra—m4: 4;. 74227.41 H NH. we; fi/Heé Mm. ‘ 4...». W745 Q.WN//€MMN (“é/gm 4.; Ha- flhfigh .55 {449,41 /’m/( .444.”sz :71 ”Cum -w—ELLH 47%;944144 4M .XN-éflen. /’//z:( 4% %.(' 4‘44 .- mm . Z22 Jwy/Q/V/fomz 44%“ /%’é:wa.m toaé.'.e WWI/n70; 4%,»? 7Vx49z<¢Lz M/eax 2: 7;...2/ 72/46 5w 1.2%; «nu/HoHH/amy’as f/fe My; M/H‘N , 542104144? ./é& £44,,“ zap/7&4 Ham” { HWLWae 25 75/52 flmrn meél. AXH//% [544,44 mxc 65444 14675.5( exp-N cod-W4 //trv7é.¢yz A47 //7/;——;.v KémH/v , éw/cif f’ém/ 566% 4m/( fivfléaée‘ 184 STUDENT NO. 2 Personal Characteristics Age: 20—24 Level of Education: Graduate Gender: Female Amount of Reading: Little Prior Knowledge Test Fluency: High Organization: Partial Combined: Partial Writing Performance Overall Language Proficiency Score: 88 Holistic Writing Score: 79 Number of Words: 340 Number of Ideas: 3 Text Sophistication: Moderate Global Cohererce: Partially coherent Involvement: Low Total Number of T—units: 29 Mean T-unit Length: 11.72 Amount of Subordination: Moderate Total Number of Revisions: 10 students' Associations to the Prompts Topic No. 3: Eclipses: The following words/phrases represent the central ideas of the topic, Eclipses. Prompt No. 1, Shadow of Eclipses: --Shadow sometimes hide the sun or the moon because the earth is moving all the time 185 Prompt No. 2, Solar Eclipses: --You can't see the sun, because the moon stays between the sun and the earth and it (the moon) (hides the light from the sun Prompt No. 3, Lunar Eclipses: --You can't see the moon, because the earth stays between the sun and the moon and it (the earth) prevents the light that shines the moon. Prompt No. 4, Astronomers: --The study of the earth, and other planets or stars Prompt No. 5, Eclipses Prediction: --The long time ago, when the reason of solar eclipses and the lunar eclipses are not known, people thought it will be the end of the earth, because the sun (or the moon) has disappeared suddenly. 186 WU?! WY! TWO INKS cl: €C-11F$QS)SO(CIC‘ €C(.‘FSC and (war QdCPse. The (chm. b coma-med uhtk twee Flam-ab) the 5mm) ‘tf‘n: C’N‘Hn Cmd “the moon. Th6 $c(0kr eclibe Occures when “We: MOON acme) 551- wiw ”on: su‘Jmcrtnn of [unar desc ) ‘Hm: eom—tw chm-urn ‘Hfl'c [:SKT ‘From The Sun ‘1'th i3 §H§nmj The, moan. \w scmz (Doer 0+ The worm.) We can not 3ee the meow; becowxse, ct this. Wage, 'WO' kmds a”? Calu‘PSeS O'CCLLNS) beccwkSe. Tke. K’QFTV‘ wad +149. (moon, rg Mex/(mg around the 5mm. U 1'37. t"3\F{"€r\) 'OhLi \'V\ '1 Let—fawn ‘Hmr, , de. H" "'9 hOT‘ 0x S+W~AR~L I‘Mbj To Malayan. Howumr) HA the, (Cw-1.3 dog; 187 0;. 0‘“ “771°” ; “MC“ The. W‘mgg M the plOmet- one, nei- Nfll—fixowmmcd and mo‘l~ decu- —G.— most of: W poem at That ‘trmc , flung Théwgtrfl‘ H- is A' k—Md 6?- ; cm 6de 0‘“ Wm fleddowhem {Mi QCUPSiS OCCMNd. EX(‘.O.M$€, The $Lm bide among 03? ‘tWL sokar QNMPSE) peopke/ chfi‘th TM») NM never Set The Sun again/J wag/‘g‘gmwd of CC'vxseiwugnuzé {'14ch UJTH (Lamar When W6 5010“" 3"?“le fixcR 04mg. firmmu‘g) These fincngs 0N ixammed .bg suavx’n‘s’vs W4?:¢Mmfid 3.0312,) awwb bacamc dear . HOLWS ‘ (VIN QMIKCK Rouwx osbcud' 90(04- (($939, and \var «1va vol/Rn Thea am in, flag, (Lennard-m3} gghccfl ) so no one‘ia‘fmx‘d a? echyses ow») more. Become, Et OCUM‘QS n01~ Mtg cfi-m/ )APQOFN -&— (001cm ”(Mex 4““? ) Per—Nara To seems ‘tws whevx (TA out/um}. wt“ ‘ ...}:— \"V is 955‘: A ow: consewu. (If ox no~+wm\ bU~T if 1‘7 C0“$id€l‘ \) 188 STUDENT NO. 3 Personal Characteristics Age: 20—24 Level of Education: Graduate Gender: Male Amount of Reading: Little Prior Knowledge Test Fluency: Partial Organization: Partial Combined: Partial Writing Performance Overall Language Proficiency Score: 71 Holistic Writing Score: 71 Number of Words: 159 Number of Ideas: 2 Text Sophistication: Moderate Global Cohererce: Coherent Involvement: Partial Total Number of T-units: 19 Mean T-unit Length: 8.37 Amount of Subordination: Low Total Number of Revisions: 1 students' Associations to the Prompts Topic No. 3: Eclipses: The following words/phrases represent the central ideas of the topic, Eclipses. Prompt No. 1, Shadow of Eclipses: ——Two kinds: Solar eclipses 8 lunar eclipses —-In that situation we can't see sun or moon. 189 Prompt No. 2, Solar Eclipses: —-We can't have sunshine --Makes dark --We can see the sun with the help of specific instruments. --It occures once in a year. Prompt No. 3, Lunar Eclipses: --We can't see the moon --It's not dangerous --It occures once in a year Prompt No. 4, Astronomers: --A people who works about astronomy. --They try to figure out mysterie of space. Prompt No. 5, Eclipses Prediction: -—It's not dangerous that's why we don't need serious prediction. 190 (71:71:: ,, .9 4w people know: domdkgf 51:54!- eah'pses Just (Gite. m€-T“~cre. are. {No has of gum... '. 55.....— ail/psts 5M4 Zumr alfpses. «1er {he éormr‘ 2.5.0992. Was, {he mean comes Beb¢% Hum AMA worH-A-L Hack Pandapkme. sonnL Psi/é 0.? earth Jaesn‘Q- MJL sonskaeflaemusc. Whom Mocks .soAslmfnLIt mew... q (“4... mmmflwaokk 3.....— QJNN P541... N.NN- .... 41x2. 54/6117“. The 0418.9.” LTAJ cg afifpsz :‘3 Last. uL‘PS-e, \K/‘nm “we. cum comes 10¢.th mooq MA wet-1A , some. PJ‘- 0‘ 141$wa mn'+ dz!" moonlifht 'm Hag racial: In “151 dried-tar) dun Made. moon-agfb‘- lunar- “flame. Lon‘lf'nuzi an Covete_ c}?- me 12,5- A5 far 51.: CL anl net'erf‘ aoncv" zclfp>¢ nOf‘ awrxr eCH-QfPsa m clawmé - frkcay OCLur" Once— 7A .1 yeflf‘ Tn NAN?» OOmC— {Dcopfle Tn ("a-e4 t NT‘H\ :41er (Duffy tk; colt“? Le ! (50m 4; {909ng try +0 See. NAN .‘1 “PM? - (They 603 aéd‘r‘olz‘ay fiCA‘l‘cr's &AQ1 M use. +¢1€5Ckopes- "\ c'O’H'JV-‘H'OA 1"" Cam be. tfi+¢re¢+fiy ( I; You out. Potash-l ‘(A ash—01:307.- REFERENCES REFERENCES Anderson, R. C. (1978). Schema-directed processes in language comprehension. In A. M. Lesgold, J. W. Pellegrino, S. D. Fokkema, and R. Glaser (Eds.), Cognitive Psychology and Instruction, (pp. 67-82). New York: Plenum Press. Anderson, R. (1984). Role of the reader's schema in comprehension, learning, and memory. In R. Anderson, J. Osborn, & R. Tierney, eds., Learning to read in American schools: Basal readers and content texts. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Publishers. Anderson, R. C., Reynolds, R. E., Schallert, D. L., and Geotz, E. T. (1977). Frameworks for comprehending discourse. American Educational Research Journal, 44, 367-382. Applebee, A. N. (1982). Writing and learning in school settings. In M. Nystrand, ed., What writers know: The language_process and structure of written discourse (pp. 365—381). New York: Academic Press. Applebee, A. N. (1984). Contexts for learning to write: Studies of secondaryyschool_instruction. Norwood, N.J.: Ablex. Bamberg, B. (1978). Composition instruction does make a difference: A comparison of the high school preparation of college freshmen in regular and remedial English classes. Research in the Teaching of English, 12, 47—59. Bamberg, B. (1984). Assessing coherence: A reanalysis of essays written for the National Assessment of Educational Progress, 1969-1979. Research in Teaching of English, 18, 305-319. Beaman, K. (1984). Coordination and subordination revisited: Syntactic complexity in spoken and written narrative discourse. In D. Tannen, Coherence in spoken and written discourse. Norwood, NJ: ABLEX. 192 193 Black, J. B., Wilkes-Gibbs, D., and Gibbs, R.W. (1982). What writers need to know that they don't know they need to know. In M. Nystrand, ed., What writers know: The language, process, and structure of written discourse. New York: Academic Press. Britton, J. (1978). The composing process and the functions of writing. In C. R. Cooper and L. Odell (Eds.), Research on Comppsing (pp. 13-29). Urbana, IL: National CounciI'of Teachers of English. Britton, J., Burgess, T., Martin, N., McLeod, A., and Rosen, H. (1975). The development of writing abilities (pp. 11-18). London: Macmillan Education. Brown, G., and Yule, G. (1983). Discourse analysis. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Calkins, L. M. (1983). Lessons from a child: On the teaching_and learning_of writing. Portsmouth: Heinemann Educational Books. Canale, M. (1983). From communicative competence to communicative language pedagogy- In J. Richard and R. Schmidt (Eds.), Language and communication (pp. 2—27). New York: Longman. Carrell, P. L. (1982). Cohesion is not coherence. TESOL Quarterly, £9! 479-488. Carrell, P. L. (1983). Some issues in studying the role of schemata, or background knowledge in second language comprehension. Reading in a Foreigg Language, 1, 81—92. Carrell, P. (1986). "Second language pedagogy in the light of schema theory." Paper presented at the 6th Annual Midwest Regional TESOL Conference, Ann Arbor, Michigan, November 6—8. Carrell, P. L. (1987). Content and formal schemata in ESL reading. TESOL Quarterly, lg, 461—481. Chesky, J .A. (1984). The effects of prior knowledge and audience on writing. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Kentucky, Lexington. 194 Chesky, J. A., and Hiebert, E. H. (1987). The effects of prior knowledge and audience on high school students' writing. The Journal of Educational Research, 89, 304-313. Chiesi, H. L., Spilich, G. J., and Voss, J. F. (1979). Acquisition of domain-related information in relation to high and low domain knowledge. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 18, 257—273. Chomsky, N. (1959). Review of Skinner's verbal behavior. Langgage, 22, 26—58. Clarke, M., and Silberstein, S. (1977). Toward a realization of psycholinguistic principles in the ESL reading class. Langgage Learning, 21, 135-154. Connor, U. (1984). A study of cohesion and coherence in English as a second language students' writing. Papers in Linguistics: International Journal of Human Communication, 11, 301-316. Cooper, A. (1988). Enhancing coherence through cohesiveness. Written Communication, g, 352-267. Coulthard, M. (1977). An introduction to discourse analysis. Longman. Crowhurst, M. (1980). Syntactic complexity and teachers' quality ratings of narrations and arguments. Research in the Teachinggof English, 14, 223-231. Cziko, G. A. (1980). Language competence and reading strategies: A comparison of first- and second- language oral reading errors. Language Learning, 3g, 101-114. DeGroff, L. (1986). The influence of prior knowledge on process-approach writing tasks. ERIC Ed 269758. Devine, J. M. (1980). Developmental patterns in native and nonnative reading agguisition. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, MiChigan State University, East Lansing, MI. Donovan, T., and McClelland, B. (1980). Eight approaches to teaching composition. Urbana, IL: National Council of Teachers of English. 195 Eamon, D. (1979). Selection and recall of topical information in prose by better and poorer reaaers. ReadingResearch_guarterly, 44, 244-257. Elley, W., Barham, 1., Lamb, H., and Wyllie, M. (1976). The role of grammar in a secondary school curriculum. Research in the Teaching of English, 10, 5-21. Eskey, D. E. (1988). Holding in the bottom: An interactive approach to the language problem of second language readers. In P. Carrell, J. Devine, and D. Eskey, Interactive approaches to second language reading (pp. 93-100). Cambridge; Cambridge University Press. Farr, R. (1969). What can be measured? Newark, DE: International Reading Association. Fish, S. (1980). Is there a text in this class? Cambridge, MA: HarvardiUniversity Press. Flower, L. S., and Hayes, J. R. (1977). Problem—solving strategies and the writing process. College English, 39, 449-461. Flower, L. S., and Hayes, J. R. (1981). A cognitive process theory of writing. College Composition and Communication, g3, 365-387. Goodman, K. (1967). Reading: A psycholinguistic guessing game. Journal of the Reading Specialist, 4, 13—26. Goodman, K., and Goodman, Y. (1982). Learning about psycholinguistic processes by analyzing oral reading. In H. B. Allen and M. Linn, ed., Readings in Applied English Linguistics. New York: Appleton—Century-Crofts. Graves, D. H. (1983). Writing: Teachers and children at work. Exeter, NH: Heinemann. Haliday, M. A. K., and Hassan, R. (1976). Cohesion in English. London: Longman. Hanania, E., and Shikhani, M. (1986). Interrelation- ships among three tests of language proficiency: Standardized ESL, Cloze, and Writing. TESOL Quarterly, 49, 97-109. 196 Hilgers, T. L. (1982). Experimental control and the writing stimulus: The problem of unequal familiarity with content. Research in the Teaching of English, 44, 381-390. Hoetker, J. (1982). Essay examination topics and students' writing. College Composition and Communication, 43, 377-392. Hudson, T. (1982). The effects of induced schemata on the "short circuit" in L reading: Non-decoding factors in L2 reading pefformance. Langgage Learning, 34, 1-31. Hunt, K. (1965). Grammatical structures written at three grade levels. Research Report No. 3. Champaign, IL: National Council of Teachers of English. Iser, W. (1978). The act of reading. Baltimore: John Hopkins University Press. Johns, A. M. (1986). Coherence and academic writing: Some definitions and suggestions for teaching. TESOLQuarterly, 4Q, 247-265. Johnson, P. (1981). Effects on reading comprehension of language complexity and cultural background of a text. TESOL Quarterly, 4;, 169-181. Jones, C. S. (1985). Problems with monitor use in second language composing. In M Rose (ed.), When a writer can't write: Studies on writer's block and other composing process problems (pp. 969—118). New York: Guilford Press. Jones, C. S., and Tetro, J. (1987). Composing in a second language. In A. Matsuhashi (Ed.), Writing in real time: Modelling the production_processes (pp. 34- 57). Norwood? APLEX. Judy, S. (1980). The experiential approach: Inner worlds to outer worlds. In D. Foster, ed., Eight approaches to teaching comppsitions: Theories, theorists, issues, problems (pp. 37-51). Boynton: Cook Publishers. 197 Kellogg, R. T. (1987). Effects of topic knowledge on the allocation of processing time and cognitive efforts to writing process. Memory and Cognition, 42, 256—266. Kincaid, G. L. (1953). Some factors affecting variations in the quality of students' writing. Unpublished Ed.D. Dissertation (Michigan State College) Michigan State University. Kitao, S. K. (1987). Schema theory: Reading and second language learning. Unpublished dissertation. Department of Communication, Michigan State University. Krashen, S. (1982). Principles and practice in second language learning. Oxford: Pergamon. Krashen, S. (1984). Writing: Research, theory, and application. Oxford: Pergamon. Langer, J. A. (1982). Facilitating text processing: The elaboration of prior knowledge. In J. A. Langer and M. T. Smith-Burke (Eds.), Reader meets Author /Bridgingithe4gap (pp. 149—162). Newark: International Reading Association. Langer, J. A. (1984). The effects of available information on responses to school writing tasks. Research in the Teaching of English, 22, 22—44. Langer, J. A., & Nicolich, M. (1981). Prior knowledge and its relationship to comprehension. Journal of Reading Behavior, 4, 373-379. Lay, N. D. S. (1982). Composing processes of adult ESL learners: A case study. TESOL Quarterly, lg, 406. Lee, J. F. (1986). The effects of three components of background knowledge on second language reading. The Modern Language Journal, 19, 350-354. Light, R. L., Xu, M., and Mossop, J. (1987). English proficiency and academic performance of international students. TESOL Quarterly, 22, 251- 261. Littlewood, W. (1984). Foreign and second language learning: Language acquisition research and its implications for classroom. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 198 Loban, W. D. (1976). Language development: Kindergarten through grade twelve. Research Report, 18, Urban, Ill: National Council of Teachers of English. McCutchen, D. (1986). Domain knowledge and linguistic knowledge in the development of writing ability. Journal of Memory and Language, 431—444. McKay, S. (1984). Some limitations in teaching composition. On TESOL '83, pp. 187—194. McLaughlin, B. (1988). Theories of second—language learning. London: Edward Arnold. Mellon, J. C. (1969). Transformational sentence combining. A method for enhancing the development of syntactic fluency in English composition. Research Report No. 10. Champaign, IL: National Council of Teachers of English. Meyers, B. F. (1985). Prose analysis: Purposes, procedures, and problem. In B. K. Britton and J. B. Black (Eds.), Understanding expository text: a theoretical and practical handbook for analyzing explanatory text (pp. 11-64). N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum. Mohan, B. A., and Lo, W. A. (1985). Academic writing r and Chinese students: Transfer and development factors. TESOL Quarterly, 22, 515-534. Mosenthal, P. (1984). The influence of classroom ideology on children's production of narrative text. American Educational Research Journal, gg, 1—28. Mosenthal, P., Conley, M. W., Collella, A., and Davidson— Mosenthal, R. (1985). The influence of prior knowledge and teacher lesson structure on children's production of narratives. The Elementary School Journal, §§, 621—634. Murray, D. M. (1978). Internal revision: A process of discovery. In C. Cooper and L. Odell (Eds.), Research on composing (pp. 85—103). Urbana, IL: National Council of Teachers of English. Nas, G. (1975). Determining the communicative value of written discourse produced by L2 learners. Utrecht, The Netherlands: Institute of Applied Linguistics. 199 Newell, G. E. (1984). Learning from writing in two content areas: A case study/protocol analysis. Research in the Teaching of English, 22, 265—287. Newell, G. E., and MacAdam, P. (1987). Examining the source of writing problems: An instrument for measuring writer' topic-specific knowledge. Written Communication, 4, 156—174. O'Hare, F. (1973). Sentence combining: Improving §tudent writing without formal_grammar instruction. Research Report No. 15. Urbana, IL: National Council of Teachers of English. Ostler, S. E. (1987). 2_study in the contrastive rhetoric of Arabic, English, Japanese, and Spanish. Unpublished dissertation, Faculty of the School of Education, University of Southern California. Pearson, P., Hansen, J., & Gordon, C. (1979). The effect of background knoweldge on young children's comprrpeehension of explicit and implicit information. Journal of Reading Behavir, 22, 201- 209. Perl, 3. (1979). The composing processes of unskilled college writers. Research in the Teaching of English, 22, 317-336. Phelps, L. W. (1985). Dialectics of coherence: Toward an integrative theory. College English, 41, 12-29. Raimes, A. (1985). What unskilled ESL students do as they write: A classroom study of composing. TESOL Quarterly, 22, 229—258. Raimes, A. (1987). Language proficiency, writing ability, and composing strategies: A study of ESL college student writers. Language Learning, 22, 439-467. Reid, J. (1987). ESL composition: The expectations of the academic audience. TESOL Newsletter, 4, p. 34. Rosenblatt, L. (1978). The reader, the text, and the poem. Carbondale, IL: Southern Illinois University Press. 200 Rumelhart, D. E. (1975). Notes on a schema for stories. In D. G. Bobrow, and A. M. Collins (Eds.), Representation and understanding studies in cognitive science. New York: Academic Press. Rumelhart, D. E. (1977). Introduction to human information processing. New York: Wiley. Rumelhart, D. (1980). Schemata: The building blocks of cognition. In R. J. Spiro, B. C. Bruce, and W. E. Brewer (Eds.), Theoretical issues in reading comprehension. HilISdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Selinker, L., Todd-Trimble, M., and Trimble, L. (1978). Rhetorical function-shifts in EST discourse. TESOL Quarterly, 22, 311-320. Sharon, A. (1972). English proficiency, verbal aptitude, and foreign students success in American graduate schools. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 22, 425-431. Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal behavior. New York: Appleton-Century—Crofts. Sommers, N. (1980). Revision strategies of student writers and experienced adult writers. College Composition and Communication, 22, 378—388. Steffensen, M., Joag-Dev, C., & Anderson, R. (1979). A cross—cultural perspective on reading comprehension. ReadingQuarterly, 22, 10—29. Stosky, S. (1983). Research on reading/writing relationships: A synthesis and suggested directions. Language Arts, 29, 627-642. Stroethoff, J. (1988). The effect of prior knowledge, prior interest, and learning on writing. Unpublished Doctoral Dissertation, Michigan State University, East Lansing, MI. Swaffar, J. K. (1988). Readers, texts, and second languages: The interactive process. The Modern Langunge Journal, 22, 123-150. Taylor, B. (1979). Good and poor readers' recall of familiar and unfamiliar text. Journal of Reading Behavior, 22, 326-329. 201 Talor, B. (1981). Content and written form: A Two—way street. TESOL Quarterly, 22, 5-12. Thomas, G. P. (1986). Mutual knowledge: A theoretical basis for analyzing audience. College English, 42, 580—594. Voss, J., Vesonder, G. T., and Spilich, G. J. (1980). Text generation and recall by high-knowledge and low-knowledge individuals. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 22, 651—667. Waltman, J. L. (1983). Grammar competency and business communications. Paper presented at the meeting of th Southwest Division of the American Business Communication Association. Houston, TX, March 10— 12. ED 227505. Wiseman, S., and Wrigley, J. (1958). Essay—reliability: The effect of choice of essay-title. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 22, 129-138. Witte, S. P., and Faigley, L. A. (1981). A comparison of analytic and synthetic approaches to the teaching of writing. TWRG Research Report No. 1. Austin: University of Texas. Department of English. ED 209677. Zamel, V. (1982). Writing: The process of discovering meaning. TESOL Quarterly, 22, 195—209. Zamel, V. (1983). The composing processes of advanced ESL students: Six case studies. TESOL Quarterly, 2_7_, 165-187. HICHIGQN STQTE UNIV. LIBRARIES I 1 \HHlle l 42fl2 ’2 312939 78