D.V'... 'I'll‘ ulfl».x;lll.ll,xl.l 1 ABSTRACT STRATIFICATION AND ALIENATION by Richard A. Brymer This thesis sought to test the relationship of social stratification to alienation, particularly average status, status inconsistency and social mobility and their bearing upon alienation. The Durkheim-Merton model of anomie was developed as an explanatory variable for the observed rela- tionship of average status to ancmie and alienation, i.e., socially structured discrepancies between goal attainment and means for achieving such goals. This rationale was then logically extended to include status inconsistency and social mobility, and hypotheses to this effect wenadevelcped. These hypotheses were then tested on a random sample of the United States gathered for this purpose in the fall of 1963. Nulti— variate analytic procedures were used to analyze the relation- ship of average status, social mobility and status inconsis- tency to alienation and its various dimensions, e.g., power- lessness, normlessness, social isolation, future orientation, and subjective assessment of one's present standing in the "goal” attainment process. Briefly, this analysis revealed the expected relationship between average status and all forms of alienation except social isolation, but no relationship between status incon- sistency, social mobility and alienation. Because of these Bryser findings, it was concluded that the Durkheim-Merton discrep- ancy rationale is not valid, and that some other theory must be developed to handle the empirically observed relationship of average status to alienation. An attespt at developing such a theory was begun, relying primarily upon the work of Herbert Gans on community organisation, and the studies of Gideon SJoberg, Richard Brymer and Buford Farris on bureauc- racy and class. 'T-w—r‘\ ly‘ STRATIFICATION AND ALIEIATION By {04 . b Richard k3 Brymer 1 THESIS Submitted to liohigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Sociology 195‘? I'M Five 5 m s'pportec' University, 1 13:11 Scrum hiorick la: ism: L. St. “Pinter, 1g ”Yuribution °f on» m“ ui Yrjo Lit ofzh. Five { 3.03180 I [Either I.“ No b. Gin uii“ dire to "91034. in. “ '03} 113mm 10: 3mm, and My“. 1 91430:.“ i. 99:303., in: P311103 AID ACKIO'LIDGIIIITS The Five Nations Project, on which this thesis was based, was supported by the International Programs of Iiohigan State university, the U. 8. Public Health Service, and the Agricul— tural Service of Iichigan State University. Its directors were Frederick.Iaisansn, Hideya Inmate, and Charles P. Lommis; Robert L. Stewart was Acting Director from September, 1963, to September, 1964. The author would like to acknowledge their contribution to this dissertation, as well as the contributions of other members of his committee. These include Donald Olmsted and Yrjo Littunen, the latter directing the Finnish Component of the live lations Project. Because much of the last chapter was written after the author left the Five Iations Project, acknowledgement must also be given to lesley Community Centers, San Antonio, Texas 'and its director, luford Farris. A special thanks is given to Iesley's Research Consultant, Gideon SJoberg. Research done at Wesley Community Centers (which contributed to this dissertation) was supported by the National Institute of Mental Health, and by the Hogg Foundation of the University of Texas. Because this dissertation is-as is much of modern sociology- the result of a conglomerate of various enterprises, acknowl- edgement is also made to all of the other known and unknown persons, including family, friends, ccuwerkers, students, etc. ii TABLE OF CONTENTS Page I. STATEMENT OF PROBLEM AND THEORETICAL POSITION . . . . 1 Introduction e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e 1 30“" of “Gui. Theory e e e e e e e e e s e e e e 5 Durkheim e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e 5 Merton e e s e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e 8 Societal ADOMIO e s e e e e e e e e e e e e e e 12 The SUhJCOtiViBtS e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e 14 ROVIO' Of Alienation Theory one s e e e e e e e e e 17 History and Early U838. e e e e e e e e e e e e 18 CUIICRt Alienation Theory e e e e e e e e e e 20 Summary of Alienation-Ancmie Theory . . . . . . . 31 Review of Empirical Alienation-Anomie Literature . 33 Operaticnalisations of Alienation-Anemia . . . 33 Inter-Correlations of Alienation Scales . . . 41 Relationships of Alienation to Other Variables . 44 Summary e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e 47 Social lbbility and Status Inconsistency . . . . . 50 8001.1 Nobility e e e e e e e e e o e e e e e e 50 Status Inconsistency . . . . o . . . . . . . . 52 Relationship of Average Status, Social nobility and Status Inconsistency . . . . . . . . . . . 58 HypothOIOB e e e e e e e e e e e s e e e e o e e 65 II. OPERATIONALIZATION OF MAJOR CONCEPTS . s e e e e e e 75 Introduction e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e 75 Average Status and Status Inconsistency . . . . . . 75 Occupation e e e e e e e e e e s e e e e e e e e 77 Education e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e 79 Income e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e 79 Ethnicity e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e 80 Average Status-Status Inconsistency . . . . . . 81 SOOill nobility e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e 82 Ali.h.t1°n e e e e e e s e e e e e e e e e e e e 84 Introduction e e e e e e e e e e e e s e e e e 84 Student Sample e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e 85 ‘dfllt $8flp1. e s e e e s e e e e e e e e e e e 88 Sub—Sample of Achieved Sample . . . . . . . . . 91 Life Satisfaction and Future Outlook . . . . . . . 92 Study COBtCIt e e e e e e e e e e e e e s e e e 94 sllpl. e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e 96 Analysis e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e 99 Introduction e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e s e 99 PTOOOdUICI e e e e e e e e e e s e e e e e e 102 Operational HypOth;l.l e e e e e e e e e e e e e e 106 iii III. SLLYSZS Introi P1: Sec Rel First Rel V a Re! I Re] I Secon: AV! St: Soc Th1: In. AV! 8:: So: Concll N.COXCLL CritL 5‘18?! Alien 3001a he Patio Cane; Res 313;:0: iv III. ANALYSIS 0 O O O O O O O O O C 0 Introduction e e e e e e e e e First Order Relationships . Second Order Relationships Relationship of Dependent Variables First Order Relationships Dependent Variables Dependent Variables Relatioaahip of Social Mobility to Dependent Variables Second Order Controlled Relationships Average Status Relationships Status Inconsistency Relationships Relationship of Average Status to Relationship of Status Inconsistency to Social Mobility Relationships . . Third Order Control Relationships Introduction Average Status . . . . Status Inconsistency Social Mobility . . Conclusions Iv. CONCLUSIONS 0 O O O O O O O Critiques of Anomie Theory Suggested Theory . . c . . Alienation and Anomie . . 300181 lbbility, e e e e e Average Status and Status Inconsistency 0.... Plth01081°&1 Behaviors e e e e e e e e e e Conclusions and Proposals for Future Research BIBLIOGRAPHY Page 111 111 113 118 124 133 133 137 145 151 151 162 167 183 183 185 192 205 218 222 223 224 232 236 240 243 248 mmnm R‘l 111-II aw . so so 00 es 0. es es 0 O. .00. see essse eeeee eseee WSWHWO ‘Se‘ 2‘31 e e e s e e e e s 0 es s as s so es e es s es as ea es .0 so so as ee as as e s IMInmhmhhhu—mhhhttdnlAfintste 5 - Base! Ale .ue‘CalAU..1s.K09\..Vsll 1‘111‘111122 e e s s e e e e e e e e e . O O O O O s s e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e O C O .1: \1J 2 I‘- 2 \J Ad I“ 'l 1' at 2 Table III. LIST OF TABLES Inter-Correlations of Types of Alienation . e e Inter-Correlations of Various Alienation Measures Distributions and Collapse Limits for Categories of Average Status and Status Inconsistency . Representativeness of Sample for Race e . e e e Representativeness of Sample for Size of Place Representativeness of Sample for Income . . . 0 Average Status by Status Inconsistency, Using MITOPVCIUORIJISOeeeeeeeeeeee Average Status by Status Inconsistency, for Collapsed values of Average Status and Status Incon‘istCnoy . o s o o e e e e s ANT“. Status by 3001.]. Mobility e e e e e Status Inconsistency by Social Mobility . . Average Status by Social Mobility at Each Love Of Status Inconsistency e e e e e e e e e e Average Status by Status Inconsistency at Rach 10.7.1 Of 3001‘]. [Ohility e e e e e e e o e e Status Inconsistency by Social Mobility at Each LC”). or Average Stfitfll e e e e e e e e e 0 Present Standing by waerlessness . . . e Present Standing by Social Isolation . . . Present Standing by Hormlessness . . s . e Present Standing by Powerlessness-Normlessness O O O O O eHeee Future Orientation by Powerlessness . Future Orientation by Social Isolation . Future Orientation by Rormlessness . . e . Future Orientation by Powerlessness-Ncrmlessne Average Status by waerlessness . e e e e . . Average Status by Social Isolation e s e e e Average Status by Normlessness . . . e e . e Average Status by Powerlessness-Normlessness Average Status by Present Standing . . e . . Average Status by Future Orientation . e e Status Inconsistency by waerlessness . e Status Inconsistency by Social Isolation e Status Inconsistency by Hormlessness e e e Status Inconsistency by Powerlessness-Rormle Status Inconsistency by Present Standing . Status Inconsistency by Future Orientation Social Robility by Powerlessness . . . . e V Page 42 42 83 98 98 99 114 116 116 118 121 121 123 126 126 127 128 130 130 130 131 134 134 135 136 138 140 140 141 141 142 143 145 146 .7 —, .7 - —-7.-? ~7’r—r-r—r-1‘r—7 23 Social 8 Sociil H Social M 80:13? Q 5031 J} Avert of‘ V Avora Lev 35 tors of 35 Aver: A1 37 Aver Le 31 Aver Le ‘3 Ave: 1 11 1‘ Ave 0 12 Ave ‘ a ‘3 Ave 1 u ‘31 45 3.} 15 3’. 17 3.“ ‘5 5. 49 S 50 S 31 S 32 E vi Table Page 28 Social Mobility by Social Isolation . . . . . . 146 29 Social Mobility by Normlessness . . . . . e . . 146 30 Social Mobility by Pewerlessness-Normlessness . 147 31 Social Mobility by Present Standing . . . . . . 148 32 Social Mobility by Future Orientation . e e . 149 33 Average Status by waerlessness at Each Level Of 300131 Mobility e e e e e e e e e e e e e 152 34 Average Status by Social Isolation at Each Level of Social nobility . . . . . . . .‘. . 152 35 Average Status by Rormlsssness at Each Level Of SOOill Mbbility e e e e e e e e e e e e e 153 36 Average Status by Powerlessness—Normlessness at EOOh LOVOI Of SOOill MObility e e e e e e 154 37 Average Status by Present Standing at Each I‘VOI Of 8001‘]. “Obility e e o e o o e o o e 155 38 Average Status by Future Orientation at Each L.V.1 or Social Mobility e e e e e e e e e e 155 39 Average Status by Powerlessness at Each Level or Social Isolation e e e e e e e e e e 156 40 Average Status by Social Isolation at Each LOVOl Of Status Inconsistency e e e e e e e e 156 41 Average Status by Rormlessness at Each Level Of st‘tu. Inconsistency o a o o o o e o o e e 157 42 Average Status by Powerlessness—Normlessness at Each Level of Status Inconsistency . . e . 158 43 Average Status by Present Standing at Each LOVOI Of Status Inconsistency e e e e e e e e 159 44 Average Status by Future Orientation at Each Level of Status Inconsistency . . . e . . . . 159 45 Status Inconsistency by Powerlessness at Each LOVOI 0f 8001‘1 Mobility e e e e e e e e e e 163 46 Status Inconsistency by Social Isolation at BEOh LOVOI Of 3001.1 Mobility e e e e e e e 163 47 Status Inconsistency by Normlessness at Each LOVIl or 300131 Mobility e e e e e e e e e e 164 48 Status Inconsistency by Powerlessness-Ncrmless- ,ness at Each Level of Social Mobility . . e . 165 49 Status Inconsistency by Present Standing at Each Level of Social Mobility . . . . . . . 166 50 Status Inconsistency by Future Orientation at B‘Oh LOVOl Of 80°1Cl Mobility e e e e e e e 166 51 Status Inconsistency by Powerlessness at Each LOVOI Of Average Status e e e e e e e e e e 168 52 Status Inconsistency by Social Isolation at ESOh LOVOI Of Average Status e e e e e e e e 169 71 Table 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 6O 61 62 63 64 65 66 67 68 69 7O 71 72 73 Status Each Status ness Status Each Status Each Social Average Status Controls for Mbbility e 0 Average Status Controls for Mobility e e Average Status Controls for Social Mobility vii Inconsistency by Level of Average Inconsistency by at Each Level of Inconsistency by Level of Average Inconsistency by Level of Average Page Normlessness at StBtUS e e e o o e e e e e 170 Powerlessness-Normless- Average Status . o s e e e 171 Present Standing at Status e e e e e e e s e e 172 Future Orientation at SLEEUS e e e e e e e . e e 772 Mobility by Powerlessness at Each Level Of Status Inconsistency e e e e e e e e e e e e 773 Social Mobility by Social Isolation at Each LOVOI Of Status Inconsistency e e e e e e e e e 173 Social Mobility by Normlessness at Each Level of Status Inconsistency . . . . . . . e . . . . 174 Social Mobility by Powerlessness—Ncrmlessness at Each Level of Status Inconsistency . . . e . 175 Social Mobility by Present Standing at Each Level of Status Inconsistency . . e . . . . . . 176 Social Mobility by Future Orientation at Each Level of Status Inconsistency . e . . . . . . e 176 Social Mobility by Powerlessness at Each Level Of Average SEOLHB e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e 177 Social Mobility by Social Isolation at Each LOVOl Of Average Status e e e e e e e e e e e s 178 Social Mobility by Normlessness at Each Level Of ‘VOIGSO Status e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e 779 Social Mobility by Powerlessness-Normlessness at Each Level of Average Status . . . . . . . . 180 Social Mobility by Present Standing at Each LOVOI Of Average Status e e e e s e e e e e e e 181 Social Mobility by Future Orientation at Each LOVOI Of Average SLILUO e e e e e e e e e e e e 787 by waerlessness Means with Status Inconsistency and Social .00000000000000000186 by Social Isolation Means with Status Inconsistency and Social e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e 787 by Normlessness Means with Status Inconsistency and 00.0000000000000188 Average Status by Powerlessness—Rormlessness Means with Controls for Status Inconsistency and Social Mobility Average Status by Present Standing Means with Controls for Status Inconsistency and Social Mobility e e e e e e e e e e e e e e 189 e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e 190 ':;e 81 :2 9 O \3 air ‘36 Table 74 75 76 77 78 79 80 81 82 83 84 85 86 viii Average Status by Future Orientation Means with Controls for Status Inconsistency and 500131 Mobility e e e e e e e e e e e e e e Status Inconsistency by Powerlessness Means with Controls for Average Status and 300131 Mobility e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e Status Inconsistency by Social Isolation Means with Controls for Average Status and 500181 Mobility e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e 0 Status Inconsistency by Normlessness Means with Controls for Average Status and $00101 Mobility e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e Status Inconsistency by Powerlessness—Normless- nose with Controls for Average Status and 800131 Mobility e e e e e e e o e e e e e e e e Status Inconsistency by Present Standing Means with Controls for Average Status and 300181 MObility e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e Status Inconsistency by Future Orientation Means with Controls for Average Status and SOOI‘I Mobility e e e e e o e e b e e e b e e e Social Mobility by Powerlessness Means with Controls for Average Status and Status Inconsistency e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e Social Mobility by Social Isolation Means with Controls for Average Status and Status Inconsistency e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e Social Mobility by Normlessness Means with Controls for Average Status and Status Inconsistency e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e Social Mobility by Powerlessness—Normlessness Means with Controls for Average Status and St‘tul InOODBiOtOnOy o e e e o o e e e e e e e Social Mobility by Present Standing Means with Controls for Average Status and Status IhOonfliltOOOI e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e Social Mobility by Future Orientation Means with Controls for Average Status and Status Inconsistency e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e e Page 191 193 195 197 199 201 203 206 208 210 212 214 216 Figure I. 1 \u 0" .... 1 Figure II. 1 \n #- UJ A) III. 1 LIST OF FIGURES Derivation of Occupation Scores . . . . . Derivation of Education Scores o . . . e Derivation of Income Scores . . . o c . . Derivation of Ethnicity Scores o . . . . Dean Items Used in Operaticnalising Alienation e e e e e e e e e o e e e e Lack of Independence in Two Multi-Variate Tables Uhing the Same Controls . . . . Two Tables Illustrating Similarity of Various Control Variables Arrangement . Page 78 79 8O 81 86 105 184 STATE In this t! Itntifioath mutton of “3 mom. effect of ,1 in th1| no 011 Film Butt “P011 111101-97: 1“ lore . Ritz-11" mi ““11 lemon, u 09631}. be “1 Van CHAPTERI STATEIEIT OP PROBIIM AID THEORETICAL POSITIOE Introduction In this thesis, our basic problem is to test the effect of stratification upon alienation. Generally, it will test an extension of the Durkheim-Merton rationale for the development of anomie. Even more generally, there is a concern with the effect of stratification upon individual behavior. Our interest in this area stems from the convergence of two areas of interest: one primarily theoretical, and the second primarily a ”hunch" based upon the observation of patterns in empirical materials. Theoretically, we have an interest in anomie and alienation, but more specifically, we are interested in the rationale that underlies the relationship of social structure to alienation and anomie. This basic rationale stems from Durkheim, via Merton, and argues that anomie is created when a discrepancy occurs between that which an individual has been led to expect, and that which he actually obtains. Anomie, in turn, leads to the creation of various types of consequences or adaptations- nearly all pathological or socially problematic. From our empirical 'hunch' source, we are concerned with the apparent overriding influence that social class has upon a large number of these quasi-pathological variables, including aliena- :‘m. That is me to am] i1il not new of lover-clue explanatory d behavior. Ania fro other Itnti 11mm, g1 3" 01 than 8“ lover “3 h"! :1 1" not ye 11111, (ac mica u The p“ ”re" c 2 tion. That is, it would appear that lower-class people are more prone to develop pathological behaviors than are others. Though it is not necessarily cited and utilised in these myriad studies of lower-class behavior, anomie theory has often been used as an explanatory device for the lower-class predilection for deviant behavior. Again from an empirical source, we have noted that there are other stratification variables—social mobility and status incon- sistency, especially—which also have been found related to a few of these more or less pathological behaviors. It would appear that lower classne ss and social mobility and status inconsistency may have something in common. Although the 'anomie' rationale has not yet been applied to status inconsistency and social mo- bility (nor tested), it would appear to be Justified from a theo- retical and an empirical stance. The purpose of this thesis will be to conjoin these two sources of interest, in an attempt to extend and test the basic 'discrepancy rationale of anomie' developed by Merton and Durkheim. Specifically, we will test the relationship of aliens- tion to status inconsistency and social mobility on the grounds that they are similar to social class, in that they exemplify the Durkheim-Merton rationale. If, in fact, status inconsistency and social mobility 31 related to alienation, then the anomie ration- ale for the relationship of stratification to 'quasi-pathological' behaviors is again validated and made more plausible. If there is no relationship, then we are left with the problem of seeking an explanation for the already proven relationship between 1:111 class intent bobs In orier nd to this b11511, in taunts, mm 33 fupter 11'. 1171107191, In use Phat, 12561: 1 111931 10 3 social class and anomie, as well as between social class and deviant behavior. In order to accomplish this task, several steps are necessary, and to this end we present an outline of the rest of the thesis. Briefly, in Chapter I we shall present our theoretical and logical arguments, ending with a statement of hypotheses. Chapter II will contain an operationalisation of these hypotheses for test. Chapter III will contain our analysis of the empirical materials developed, and Chapter IV will contain our conclusions. In more detail, Chapter I will proceed as follows: First, we shall review anomie theory as it has developed with Durkheim and been extended by Merton. In this discussion, we shall attend to the distinction betwun anomie as a societal condition, and anomie—or enemy—as an individual condition. Although Durkheim and Merton focused ostensibly upon anomie as a societal condition, there is, we believe, a rationale implicit in their theories which can be used to account for anemia as an individual condition. To this end, we shall also review those writers who are interested in anomie as an individual condition. Second, we shall review and discuss alienation theory in an attempt to illustrate a convergence between anomie and alienation, in terms of their general definitions, as well as antecedents and consequences. This does not constitute an essentially original effort, as many sociologists consider alienation and anomie to be identical or synonymous terms for the same phenomena. Although many theorists view alienation as having a slightly different theoretical tradition, we shall treat alienation separately, and :hen attempt t1 Third, we 32 studies of hot tunsmte th 111111111 at 1'11 theoretic: dieution ant 1.1m miabl mi unit. 1011 concert W conseque '9 331311 be I 33131110!“ 11 mm Class- Fourth, , “tend the I ”W Iohi] fin: mom 1111338 111 than ‘. “a 1‘, 1. ""3 11100 4 then attempt to illustrate a convergence. Third, we shall review the literature concerned with empirical studies of both anomie and alienation. we shall (a) attempt to demonstrate that there is considerable overlap between alienation and anemia at the empirical and operational levels, as well as at the theoretical levels; and (b) explore the relationships of alienation and anemia to other variables in order to show that these variables are stably and consistently related to alienation and anemia. Generally, there will be two orders of studies; those concerned with antecedent conditions and those concerned with consequences of alienation-anomie. In the former instance, 'we shall be particularly concerned with the very stable and consistent relationship between alienation and anemia, and social class-however operationalised. Fourth, and following the above discussion, we shall then extend the DurkheiméMerton rationale to two additional areas—- social mobility and status inconsistency. In so doing, we shall first demonstrate the theoretical and logical rationale for con- sidering these two variables as examples of the DeM rationale; then we shall present a review of the empirical literature for status inconsistency and social mobility, in which we will demon— strate that they are related to a series of variables which have also been termed possible consequences of alienation. In the fifth and final section, we shall present a summary of our contentions so far, and on this basis, suggest a series of hypotheses stipulating a relationship of average status, social mobility and status inconsistency to alienation. ‘yv‘l”. fl: 3 hon. mm I integrati: integrati High “$9 mi 1 liailu-i to 601:0 $9381 : mutt: Review of Anomie—Anomia Theory Durkheim Anomie theory began, in the sociological tradition, with Durkheim and his interest in the mechanisms of societal unity and integration. In his classic work, The Division of Labor in Society, Durkheim distinguishes between two general types of integration-mechanical andorganic.1 In simple societies, with a simple division of labor, persons are differentiated only by age and sex and have a great deal in common. Because of this similarity, a form of integration can be achieved by reference to common ideas, interests, etc. This type of integration is termed.mechanical and is characteristic of simple societies. In industrial societies, however,'with their complex division of labor and attendant specialisation of function and personnel, persons are not likely to have a great deal in common, and, therefore, a different form of integration develops, based upon mutual interdependence. The development of this interdependence- or organic integration—requires a period of lengthy contact and mutual adjustment of groups which is not required in simple societies. It would seem that Durkheim regarded the development of integration in industrial societies as more problematic and tenous than in simple societies. This notion is further evi- denced by Durkheim's attention to the forms of abnormal inte- gration (or specifically, abnormal divisions of labor) which may develop-particu1arly in industrial societies. It is in the description of one of these abnormal forms that the use of the “:1 1:10:10 1 which the: htordoponder w. pm e If Dubai: did «the loci. In hie YO imam-.1 q «n am. if (Our ‘7? mm: in int-c, Ogoj ‘h' Iuicid. ti“ the; 3110. In “P5331: \ ”Hider. 6 term anomie first occurs. Anomie was a form of division of labor in which there had not been extended contact, so that mutual interdependence had not developed. Although the term anomie did not play a significant part in this work, it is clear that Durkheim did use it to characterise and refer to a state of the entire society or group. In his volume, Sgicide, Durkheim attempts to formulate social structural explanations of variations in suicide rates; here the term anomie becomes significant.2 In this volume, anomie is one of four types of abnormal societal conditions which lead to in- creases in suicide rates. Frequently these terme—-anomic, fatal- istic, egoistic and altruistic-—have been used to characterise the suicide itself, although it would seem that Durkheim intended that they refer to the social conditions which produced the sui- cide. In order to understand Durkheim's use of anomie, his emphasis upon the functions of norms in society must also be considered. Durkheim viewed human nature as essentially uncontrolled, greedy and avaricious. Ian was a biological creature who, without the presence of external controls, would seek to fulfill his needs far beyond.the point of simple satisfaction. ”Human activity naturally aspires beyond assignable limits and sets itself un- attainable goals."3 ‘Iith this view’of human nature, Durkhemm saw society (in particular, its normative structure) as furnishing the necessary controls. lcrms specify the goals that man can achieve, and what he must do to achieve them. Dy specifying goals, norms both limit and Justify man's activity. In addition, the 3mm 0! t1:- uut the mm mm. “Sac? condition, en it moral re uquilibriun 1 my Iilhdp POM!!! e I think ute ‘ Mi thereby him. .1 lemme ‘3 “tel-en ‘ lien in “In: “on to l 1:: ms. "1:311I a! “in? “In“ v! u“ “J 7 presence of these norms allows the individual to make predictions about the future behavior of others, and himself in relation to others. ”Each person is then, at least . . . in harmony with his condition, and desires only what he may legitimately hope for as the normal reward of his activity. He has the essentials. The equilibrium of his happiness is secure because it is defined, and a few’mishaps cannot disconcert him."4 The normative structure provided a stable frame of reference and meaning for human beings, ‘which sets their goals, defines what must be done to achieve them, and thereby establishes an orderly, predictable endeavor. Durkheim even suggested that poverty, as long as it is viewed as legitimate and according to the norms, provides a meaningful frame of reference.5 L.disruption of the normative structure, however, also disrupts the meaningfulness and predictability of human life, and contrib- utes to a state of tension, confusion, and loss of orientation. Ian presumably reverted to his original state, and his aspirations rapidly rose beyond any expectations of fulfillment. This state of affairs-hthe absence of norms-whioh produced continual frus- tration, anxiety, pointlessness, etc.,'was termed anomie. Further, the anemic situation‘was seen as intolerable for any length, so that anomie suicide resulted. Durkheim saw many sources of anomie in industrial society—- depression, inflation, unexpected prosperity, divorce, and upward and downeard.mebility. The general mechanism which underlies these diverse situations is that the norms, as guides specifying“what one must do in order to achieve a certain goal, no longer operate. Intho cm of :rditioml nc me of prospe homily oxpec mqun. tinl: paler logitincy an mm, end I ”dilution .1 In um”: “Flutter: . manned t P! u, u , “ion. in ll 31. “1° r1 "Minion min '91.. .c‘e, mbi] 8 In the case of an economic depression, when one follows the traditional norm, one no longer gets the expected goal. In the case of prosperity, one obtains far more than he would have normally expected. In both cases, there is a considerable die- crepancy between the expectations and attainments that were norma- tively prescribed. This discrepancy casts doubt upon both the legitimacy and.the efficacy of the norm itself. It is no longer useful, and apparently discarded. But without it, there is no regulation at all, so that human life eventually becomes confused. In summary, Durkheim.developed the concept of anomie as an explanatory device which operated at the societal level, and influenced the suicide rate. His main concern was not with anomie per so, as experienced by the individual, but more with the vari- ations in the suicide rate and the social conditions themselves. His basic rationale for the individual was a discrepancy between expectations and achievements, and particularly as these discrep- ancies were produced by changes in an individual's social position, e.g., mobility, divorce, etc. 2.2m Robert I. Merton, in his seminal article, "Social Structure and inc-is," has both extended and generalised Durkheim's theory of anomie.6 He has extended it in the sense that he sought to explain many forms of 'deviant' behavior rather than just suicide, and generalised it in that the sources of anomie and consequent deviant behavior are seen, not as consequences of the breakdown of norms, but as inherent in the social structure itself. In a way, ‘lerton was even more sociologically oriented.than Durkheim, for he iidnot sake t m that Durkh all a his do in, 1..., er I: a locally In dmlop: htmn cultu: in dim-0pm ”mixed at ‘° Ilbm of “1m“ Itn “0 Mum. linen” 1. i"hick th. 9 did not make the assumptions about the "biological” nature of man that Durkheim did. Rather, he saw man's aspirations, as well as his deviant behavior, as a product of the social struc- ture, i.e., anomie not as a reversion to a "natural state”, but as a socially induced product. In developing his theory of anomie, Horton distinguishes between cultural and social structure, with anomie as a product of the discrepancy between them. Cultural structure is ". . . that organised set of normative values governing behavior which is common to members of a designated society or group."7 That is, the cultural structure contains the values and goals of the society- the definitions of things that man must strive for. Social structure is the " . . . organised set of social relationships in which the members of the society or social groups are vari- ously implicated."8 Thus, social structure is the set of insti- tutional norms or means which is specified in order to reach the culturally defined goals. The social structure and the cultural structure are, however, independent of each other and ". . . the cultural emphasis placed upon certain goals varies independently or the degree of emphasis upon institutionalised msans.”9 inomie, then, is ”. . .a breakdown in the cultural structure, occurring particularly when there is an acute disjunction between cultural norms and goals and the social structured capacities of members 10 of the group to act in accord with them." Anomie refers to a condition of the society, although the members of the society are affected by it, since it is they who fail to achieve the goals that they have been enjoined to seek. This anomie, once it has come into existence m arm" I mall: some ; [man devote fuiliu- eds; M1" the I .3811! (or be "11" mm. mung}; misty or mi“): N: “trim; c, “fluted . 1“1! Gone “trio“ ‘ “i the c 99:53“ 1 far 1:33 1°th ’ 4‘s. 10 into existence, sets up a strain towards equilibrium, which in turn creates a form of adaptation.11 These adaptations are usually some form of deviant behavior, and it is to them that Ilerton devotes the majority of his emphasis. Generally, the now familiar adaptations, as laid out by Horton in his original work, involve the acceptance or negation of either the goals or the means (or both), or the creation of a new set of both goals and means. Although.lsrton's schema logically could be applied to any society or group, he devoted much of his analysis to American society, and, in particular, to the discrepancy between the American cultural goal of success, and the differentially dis- tributed social means for attaining such a goal. He was partioup larly concerned with the discrepancy between the egalitarian American ethic which held ”success" to be within the reach of all and the class system of America which encumbered lower-class persons in the struggle for success. This mechanism would account for the disproportionate representation of lower—class persons in deviant groups . In summary, Merton seems to attend only to the antecedent conditions, and the consequences of anomie, with very little attention paid to anomie, per so. Also, Merton seems to be ccncsrnednwith anomie as a societal condition. Although he recognises the possibility of researching the subjective aspects of anemia, and even cites Srols's and lacIver's work, it is clear from the amount of attention he devotes to the topic that he is 12 primarily interested in anemia as a societal condition. The ntiomlo for objective as min, Iilila mid hold t1 teal soul a «9": the gen structure a‘. If he does - that create an inn“; sizing, h 'riterg. madly thou 'hc "fare“. of °Pfira 11 rationale for the development of 'individual' anomie, or the subjective aspects of anomie, seems to be implicit in Merton, and again, similar to those of Durkheim. Generally, this rationale would hold that the individual must first be exposed to the cul- tural goal and, to some degree, internaliss it; then, he will seek the goal to the extent that his position in the social structure allows it; and, if he achieves it, conformity results. If he does not achieve it, a discrepancy will exist, which will then create anomie and the need for some sort of adaptation (for the individual). Although the rationale is present in Merton's writing, he has done little with it. This has remained for other writers. For those pcst—Itsrton sociologists, there seem to be generally two schools of thought and effort. First, there are those who utilise a more or less societal or group frame of reference. These persons have attempted to develop new methods of operationalising societal anomie, and extend the Hertonian theory. Second, there are those scholars who have taken a sub- jectivist approach and are attempting to discover the correlates and conditions of the subjective aspects of anomie—or anomia as their group generally prefers to call it.13 Although we have somewhat arbitrarily separated these two approaches, we do not mean to imply that they have had no over- lap. Psrhaps much of the overlap is only at an operational level in research. Inch of the difficulty in drawing a clear-cut line between these two approaches stems from the form that their research takes. lany who would classify themselves as interested in societal anomie utilise data gathered on individuals and their numeric infining an the theory Ibout, loci of individ‘. overlap. I lnic purp: mmch a “Poet: of Societal A Sine. I main the basic rgtf 10m), b 'it‘n u. "my bet. 13m. to: D'dbin’ 12 characteristics. And conversely, many who use attitude scales— defining anomia as an individual state of mind—both cite and use the theory developed by persons who are interested in, and writing about, societal anomie. And no doubt, the old theoretical devil of individual versus group contributes no small degree to this overlap. Is will, however, arbitrarily separate them for ana- lytic purposes, because the author feels that there is a closer research affinity betwun the persons studying the subjective aspects of anomie. Societal Anomis Since Horton, there have been several contributions to the anomie theory. The majority of these, however, deal not with the basic rationale for anomie (either at the group or individual level), but more with the adaptations stealing from anomie, or with the particularistic social situation in which the discrep- ancy betwaen the cultural goals and the socially structured means for achieving them occurs. Dubin, for example, subdividss Horton's category of means into two categories: means and norms.14 loans are those actual behav- iors that a person carries out in his daily activities. lorms specify what is prescribed behavior and what is prescribed behav- ior. This distinction sets actual behavior apart from the values used by actors to select among the behavioral choices. Iith this new distinction, Dubin expands the lsrtonian adaptation categor- ies of Innovation and Ritualism from one to six alternatives each. Rebellion and ritualism are retained as in the original work. Tu Iertonian (show) i Clonrd, . nay (ulthou than in a 1 ta and i :11: mum am the - um, 01 file ma 1 3°11 of ' then 11] fleas. . “not“ “ultra "that “will: hi‘ ha' 13 The Hertcnian rationale of a discrepancy between norms, goals (and.means) is not, however, modified. Cloward, on the other hand, has extended the notion of discrep- ancy (although he has not basically modified it) by noting that there is a socially structured differential access to both legit- imate and illegitimate means.15 That is, if one cannot attain the culturally prescribed goals by legitimate means, there is still the possibility that he may attain them by illegitimate means. Oloward suggests that where illegitimate means are avail- able and the person 'sueoseds' in using them, there is a different form of 'innovation' than where the person does not succeed in these illegitimate structures and.must create his own innova- tions. Of course, if he cannot gain access to the illegitimate structure, nor create one of his own, then a situation of double failure exists, and no form of adaptation is left but that of the rstreatist. Although Oloward was specifically interested in applying and.rsvising the anomia theory to deliquent behavior, his actions regarding the necessity of inspecting'means struc- tures, other than the traditional institutional structure, have been the point of departure for other critiques and reformulations of the lertonian theory of anomie. The majority of the other reformulations of’ancmie theory generally see the need for adapting the notion of goals-means to the particular social situation or group in which the actor is operating. Short, for example, stresses the need for attending to the goals and means present in the delinquent gang itself, rather than assuming that the societal goals and means are null] “lie: umption of «mom, and cultural gm betmn the In luau} touched the i! not to ‘ m been u ‘M then “Plicitll taken int. 14 equally salient for all persons.“ Iemsrt notes that the assmpticn of society-wide goals in a pluralistic society is tenuous, and attention should be paid to the goals of these sub- cultural groupings, especially as they mediate and intervene between the individual and the societal levels." In summary, these post-Hertonian theoretical changes have not touched the underlying rationale whereby anomie develops. This is not to say that criticisms of the discrepancy rationale have not been advanced, but within the tradition of anomie theory per so, there have been none, nor in the research that has stemmed explicitly from it have there been these sorts of criticisms taken into account, as we shall see.18 Th3 Sfljsgt 13st! Ry subjectivists, we do not mean to refer to those persons who are interested in ascertaining the psychological bases or ants- oedsnts of anomia. Rather, we refer to those who have an interest in anomia as a characteristic of an individual; as a set of beliefs about the nature of the world that he perceives; and as an individ- ual stats of mind. lost writers in this tradition, as implied above, are explicitly sociological in that they see the cause of individual attitudes, etc., as stemming from the individual's position in society.‘9 That is, the stats of societal anomie is held to create a parallel state for the individual. Perhaps the first two sociologists to become interested in anomia were KacIver and Srole, in 1950. hacIver, for instance, is very explicit in his concern over the individual aspects of mom. holy signi: up by hit a only diooon continuity, boon spin unponnibl: nonunion ‘ state of a minion- fatnlly we Clearly, t] on the other theoretical titugung ‘ mm.- 2‘ mm“ 1: “ mm 13117111141: “‘3‘ int. 15 anomia. Anomy signifies the state of mind of one who has been pulled up by his moral roots, who has no longer any standards, but only disconnected urges, who has no longer any sense of continuity, of folk, of obligation. The anosic man has become spiritually sterile, responsive only to himself, responsible to no one . . . He lives on the thin line of sensation between no future and no past . . . Anomy is a state of mind in which the individual's sense of social cohesion-the mainsgring of his morals-is broken or fatally‘seakened. 2 Clearly, this is a subjectivist conception of anomie. Srcle, on the other hand, is much less a subjectivist-at least, at the theoretical level. In his early works, he states that he is attempting ” to test hypotheses centering on Durkheis's concept of “0.1.e. 21 He notes that although Durkheim focused mainly on the societal level, there is the possibility of a ”parallel continuum of variations seen from the 'microscopic' or molecular view of individuals as they are integrated in the total action fields of their interpersonal relationships and.reference groups."22 Further, he argues that there can exist a continuum ranging from 'eunomia' to 'anonia', with the former ”referring to the individ- ual's generalised, pervasive sense of 'self-to-others belonging- ness' and [with] 'self—to—others distance' and 'self-to—cthers alienation' at the other pole of the continuum.” 23 Little more theoretical attention is given to the development of his conceptu- alization. However, in his rationale for cpsrationalisation, we get an even clearer cue that Srole considers his anomia conceptuali- saticn to refer to "the ideaticnal states or components that on on theoretical grounds would represent‘igtggggliggg.counterparts or reflections, in the individual's life situation, of conditions of mini $331 Beaming ' ntion, Srolo of Karton the mint mil ntian in the (FM!) or a hnthcr, Sr: hm ac: ”Gratin “tunic 1. Refer 'ithin I 2. Gene: con' 5“]- cm and (c) 3. Th. tionghi “him “one, ttrucm 16 of social dysfunction.”24 Regarding the sources of this anomia or eelf-to-others alien- ation, Srole appears to follow the explicit sociological tradition of llerton when he states that "social dysfunction is the inde- pendent variable, the individual's state of self-to—group alien- ation is the intervening variable, and change in personality (Fromm) or adaptive modes (Herton) is the dependent variable”?5 further, Srole explains: Three more inclusive sets of forces are also seen as operating in his [the anomia person] contemporary situation. 1. Reference groups beyond his immediate field of action, within which acceptance and ultimate integration are sought. 2. Generalised qualities of the molar society netrating his contemporary action field as these affect a) his life goal choices, (b) his selection of means toward these goals, and (c) his success or failure in achieving these goals. 3. The scoialisation processes of his interpersonal rela- tionships during child hood and adolescence, as these have conditioned the interpersonal expectations, value orienta- tions, and behavioral tendencies of his current personality structure. 25 Perhaps Srcle's greatest influence in this area was his devel- opsent of the now famous Irole Scale for measuring individual anomia. Following the development of this scale, there have been few purely theoretical treatises on the nature and sources of individual anomia. On the other hand, there has been a virtual explosion of empirical studies, each making their own contribution to our knowledge about anomia. In explaining this explosion, it is worthy to note the ease cfzus'in‘g an attitude scale versus the difficulty of constructing group indexes. Primarily, these studies have sought to correlate anomia with conditions which might be thought to exemplify the Hertcnian rationale, and they will be (1180' spiricnl 11 In sums: uonin theoz the nuntnint Ind it: mm. M mnderi Inbntlntivq fl n1» lg: 'mtually ”filmtorl Winn“ o “1' 'hich hit”. in mm a! it . . “‘4 up) in our r1 “Mich inn 1121 Eve: 01 “I. 17 will be discussed in the section dealing with a review of empirical literature. In summarising our perusal of selected writers in anomie- anomia theory, we are forced to agree with Cohen. ”In view of the sustained interest in anomie theory, its enormous influence and its nmerous [empirical] application . . . it is worth noting and wondering at the relatively slow and fitful growth of the substantive growth theory itself." 27 In addition, we find that we also agree with IcOlosky and Schaar when they say that ”virtually all of this [empirical] work has employed a single explanatory model for the analysis of ancmyc a specified social- cultural condition gives rise to specified feelings in individ- uals which in turn result in specified behaviors. Different writers have werked variations on this scheme, but nobody has challenged the scheme it self or attempted a fundamental revision of it . . . [and] virtually all studies of ancmy have employed the 28 Is must note, however, as we shall see a explanatory model.” in our review of empirical literature, that no study using an explicitly avowed anemic theory framework has come up with empizb ical findings which have directly challenged anomie theory. Therefore, the explanatory rationale appears to hve some valid- it’s Review of Alienation Theory 'e have arbitrarily separated alienation theory from anomie theory because alienation theory seems to have a much longer history as well as a wider scope of discussion, at least in its me by soc the uin, al eociety' vie the etructun the ho cone introductio- hner‘e v01- etiented 31 ntien thee: intereeted Of social 1 intended a: ti”; nth. cm: 6133 18 usage by social critics from a wide variety of intellectuals. In the main, alienation is used by those theorists holding to a ”mass society" viewpoint, whereas anomie-anomia theorists are more from the structural-functional point of view. is we shall see, however, the two concepts are similar. Is shall first provide a brief introduction to this history of the concept, relying heavily upon Peuer's work.29 Then we shall turn to those more empirically oriented alienation theorists forthe bulk of our review of alien- ation theory. We shall confine our attention to those theorists interested in developing the theory of alienation for the purposes of social reseu-ch—rather than social criticism. This is not intended as a slight to social critics nor to demean their func- tion; rather it is a recognition of a distinction between social criticism and social science. History 2d larly Hem According to Peuer, alienation is an ethical concept having its historical source in ”the youthful larx who in manuscripts, sometimes, unpublished, erte down an ethical critique of capi- talism."3o Peuer argues that Harx picked up the term and its accompanying vocabulary from Hegel, who in turn ”imbibed the concept of alienation from pessimist Protestant theology (Calvin, in particular)."31 Generally, Marx saw alienation as a condition resulting from the separation of the worker from the product he made as well as the means of production. Or, at least this is the traditional contemporary interpretation of llarx's meaning of alienation. Peuer maintains, however, that this is a super— fiml reniin first need by Ina Fonerbach semi comet epnoteet of civilisation, pence vith bo toneept which Uthough r the tea has nliennted int 31”“! lkin ”leucel co: ‘hwriete. 1 “3 Minot 31““ View: miaicle. Fe “.1, "1d co Weep: of 19 ficial reading of Marx's theory, and that "...'a1ienation' as first used by Marx, Engels, and their fellow young Hegelians and Peuerbachians was a romantic concept, with a preponderantly sexual connotation. It was the language of a group which made a protest of romantic individualism against the new capitalist civilisation, but which soon went on to its post-adolescent peace with bourgeois society. Marx and Engels discarded a concept which became alien to their own aims.”32 Although rejected by Marx and Engels, and classical Marxists, the term has returned to our vocabulary via MacDonald, and other alienated intellectual social critics of the 1930's vintage. Closely akin to the social critics‘ usage of alienation as a polemical concept has been the use of the term by ”mass society” theorists. lass society theorists view alienation as a slightly evil product of modern society, somewhat in the manner that Simmel viewed modern society.33 In this style, alienation is sometimes referred to as a cause of everything from apathy to suicide. Peuer is somewhat critical of this wide usage of the term, and concludes that ”...'alienaticn' remains too much a concept of political theology which bewilders rather than clari- fies the direction cf political action...[is] a dramatic metaphor which for reasons peculiar to intellectual's experience has become their favorite root-metaphor for perceiving the social universe... [and] a projection of the psychology of intellectuals disenchanted Vi th themselves . " 34 4 went the lettler, he connente tron anomie tion.35 let he been eat the culture minty."36 We of th. °°3°°Ption c 'ith reel little: an critic, (1,5. lily. '6 I ”muons: ‘50 “Imp “hm-‘1 y; 20 ' Current Alienation Theory lettler, in an effort to clarify the referents of alienation, has commented upon the degree to which alienation is distinct from anomie (as a societal condition) and personal disorganisa- tion.35 Nettler's definition of an alienated person is "one who has been estranged from, made unfriendly toward, his society and the culture it carries ... the feeling of estrangement from society."36 lxplicitly, this definition refers to a psychological state of the individual. Nettler appears to equate Srcle's conception of anemia with alienation, however. Iflth.respect to the conditions under which.alienation occurs, lettler cites a large number of the mass society theorists and critics (Proms, De Grasie, etc.) but contributes little person- ally. ‘Ie may assume, however, from his theoretical guide to operaticnalisation—-i.e., "all specific questions were guided by the assumption that the alienated person would £32.22}. the ccmon cultural values of his society”31-that alienated persons reject the cultural goals in favor of their own. Vb would assume that this is at least not incongruent with the Merton rationale of a discrepancy, although how Rattler would feel about the rejection or acceptance of norms is obviously not known. Theoretically, there is another alternative interpretation of lettler's position. Given that his definition of alienation includes rejection of cultural goals, and estrangement from the rest of society, it may be logically possible to consider this as a form of Horton's 'retreatist' adaptation. In this case, the antecedents of lettler's ”alienation” again could logically be congruent‘with letton'e no: or elienntic qnencee of I mid negate One of t' wntuporu- u inepecti in dienati one of th. llh the t; '11”? IP11 thwflticaj 5‘93 used.‘ 21 ‘lerton's conceptualization of the preconditions of anomie- or alienation. Regarding the theoretically possible conse- quences of alienation, lettlsr says little, but apparently 'wonld regard it as an open question, to be answered empirically. One of the most productive writers and researchers on the 38 This is evident from ccntemporary scene is Kelvin Seaman. an inspection of the institutional affiliations of many writers in alienation.39 In an effort to "make more organised sense of one of the great traditions in sociological thought ... and to make the traditional interest in alienation more amenable to sharp»empdrical statement," Seeman has developed five basic, but theoretically separable, ways in which the term alienation has “.n “.4e4o The five variants arse (1) poserlessness or ”the expectancy or probability held by the individual that his own behavior cannot determine the occurrence of the outcomes, or reinforcements, he seeks”;41 (2) meaninglessness, or the "low expectancy that satisfactory predictions about future outcomes of behawior can be made”;42 (3) normleesness, or a ”high expect- ancy that socially unapproved behaviors are required.to achieve given goals”;43 (4) isolation, or the condition in which the person ”assignEs] low reward values to goals or beliefs that are typically highly valued in the given society”;44 and (5) self- estrangement, or “the degree of dependence of the given behavior upon anticipated future rewards".45 All variants are explicitly individual states, and, maintains Seeman, should not be confused or equated with personal disorganisation, or other quasi-patho- logical chars oliosetion, t moo! defini etion, in the oble. to inspect cf nlienetlor with miounl etion. H. r Rotter: [Rotter' d'POnde the beh Elk of H these b'haViorl Neon q when I likely e 22 logical characteristics. Although these phenomena may accompany alienation, they are not identical with it. Seaman's relatively narrow definitions of alienation are an aid in researching alien- ation, in that they make alienation somewhat more Operationalis- able. An inspection of Seaman's writings with respect to the causes of alienation seems to indicate that mass society, in conjunction with various aspects of learning in a mass society, create alien- ation. He relies very heavily upon the social learning theory of Rotter: [Rotter's] principal contention is that human behavior 1 depends on ) the degree to which a person ex cts that the behavior will have a successful outcome, and (2) the value of that success to the person trying to achieve it. If these factors are powerful, separately or together, the behavior is most likely to occur. Specifically, if a person expects that learning somethinngill help him achieve some goal and/or he values that goal, he is more likely to learn.‘ Another factor influencing learning is the degree to which a person conceives cf the success or failure of a given behavior being due to an external or an internal factor, i.e., something outside his control or under his control. ”A person will definitely learn‘;ggg_from experiences he conceives to be domi- nated by outsiders, cr-by chance, which he feels he cannot influ- 47 I This general learning theory is, in Seaman's view: Parallel [to] the argument of the followers of mass theory that the isolated individual in the 'lonely crowd', sub- ordinated to and intimidated by bureaucracy, becomes con- ‘vinced of his powerlessness and gives up learning about those things that might affect his future. As a specific example, he becomes apathetic and igdifferent to politics . . . 'You can't fight city hall'.4 In genera mote eltue ontoonee of menu: in notes lain‘ iniividual 1 his create hie eet min of t to the burg "tire with :More 1. hot they. ”Rx-01’ O! h 'Ould Is 23 In general, Sesman maintains that bureaucratic social systems create situations where persons cannot learn how to control the outcomes of their behavior, i.e., they cannot learn what is necessary in order to obtain a reward. Or at least that bureauc- racies maintain control over the reward systems so that what the individual does has no bearing upon the rewards that he receives. This creates the powerlessness, normleesness, etc. This set of propositions does not, however, contain a specifi- cation of the particular form of the relationship of the individual to the bureaucracy. And unless we can assume thatial;.relation- ships with bureaucracy produce alienation, then some other set of factors is necessary. Additionally, Seeman's view seems to assume that there is, on the part of the individual, some expectation of control, or at least, reward.49 If this assumption is made, then it'would seem that Seeman's learning theory-mass society rationale is not basically incompatible with the Durkheimélertcn rationale, but more an explication and extension of it for the 'mass society' context. That is, given the nature of bureaucratic systems, there is a distinct possibility that the individual will experi- ence a discrepancy between his desire for control, meaning, rewards, etc., and the control, meaning, rewards, etc., that he actually achieves. In this manner, then, we can consider Seeman's theoretical rationale as a part of the discrepancy rationale. As we shall see later,‘Iaisanen has explicitly set out a series of 'discrepancy' conditions which can lead to Seaman‘s various forms of alienation.50 Seeman suggests many possible consequences of alienation. lilienetion 11 political pan out in seven hm little 1 mm! his I dienntion p lend to . fa 1’ " are ac Womtion lent to a refomtou ‘53 tart.E the lore ; "tion u, . naming ““1? or , “51‘" ll: “lion, & ‘é’tin, it 24 ”Alienation in turn results in al enated behavior, such as political passivity, racial and religious prejudice, taking part in movements that promise to usher in the millenium (but have little immediate or practical effect) and the like."51 Perhaps his most provocative consequence is the degree to which alienation prohibits the learning of new information that would lead to a form of control by the individual over his situation. If we are not misinterpreting Seaman, learning certain types of information about a situation lead to control over it, or at least to a sense of control. But in his study of hospitals and reformatcries, he notes that alienation precludes learning of this sort.52 Therefore, alienation may be spoken of as beget- ting’more alienation—-or at least alienation sets up a situation ‘which may not be self-correcting, and is likely to be ever increasing. Somewhat the same rationale is presented in his study of unions and powerlessness, where he argues that to some degree alienation is both a structural result of lack.of partici- pation, and a activation 59.; 3g belong to organisations.53 And again, it has the consequences of feeding upon itself, and increasing. In general, Seeman's contributions seem to lie in the area of narrowly specifyingHwhat alienation is, so that it may be more sharply distinguished from its antecedents and its consequences. And although he relies upon a relatively general theory of mass society as an antecedent condition, it does not appear to be in conflict with the 'discrepancy' rationale set forth by Durkheim and Herton. Glut, who Sum, oxpli ad in part1: tha diacropa: 1mm the to whine t “961in sit b0 i loam-q he has, and I“ 1110 r14 1' efident (and there: Munoz-at: ":3 that a mm“ in < "Did Ihif- lion to ‘ ”when TDBBQ d 25 Clark, who also would appear to have been influenced by Seaman, explicitly illustrates the possibility that alienation—- and.in particular powerlessness-may indeed stem from a form of the discrepancy between expectations and achievements.54 He defines alienation as ”the degree to which man feels powerless to achieve the role he has determined to be rightfully his in specific situations."55 Further, ”a measure of alienation must be a measure of the discrepancy between the power man believes he has, and what he believes he should have-his estrangement from.his rightful role."56 Although not explicitly stated, it is evident that Clark feels that such a situation of discrepancy (and therefore alienation) is more likely to develop in a large bureaucratic situation. In characterising his study site, he says that "this cooperative illustrates one of the most striking trends in cooperative organisations in recent years, namely the rapid shift from a small, community-centered, personal organiza- tion to a large, sprawling, more complicated, and.impersonal organisation."57 These definitions and comments indicate that Clark feels that alienation is an individual state which results from a discrepancy between cne's expectations and achievements, which is very likely to occur in.mass societies. 'With respect to the consequences of alienation Clark has little to say. Following Seeman's lead in defining alienation, Dean has attempted to develop empirical evidence regarding the separability of the various types of alienation.58 Although Dean cites Seeman, and follows his sub-types, there is a good bit of divergence Moon the t approxintoly inability to Mined by I) am ind m! hclnr‘. t “he: so t The mnflic 'incormm. "Maris “1mm: lifinE Vc‘. ”01:1,, ‘ 3mm 1 bank“ m. high ‘ Elven ct331301163 in: 1 ”‘68! 26 between the two. Both Dean and Seeman define powerlessness in approximately the same manner, as a state of helplessness, of inability to control one's future. Normlessness, however, is defined by Dean as consisting of two sub-sub-typesa purposeless- ness and conflict or norms. Purposelessness is similar to lacIver's anomy-a state in which the individual has lost his values so that he has no guiding purposes or goals in his life. The conflict of norms occurs, for example, when the individual ”incorporates in his personality conflicting norms such as the standards of Christianity versus the success imperative, the stimulation toward a constantly higher material standard of living versus the practical denial of a high standard for many people, and the alleged freedom of the individual versus the factual limitations on his behavior."59 Both of these types of normleesness are at variance‘with Seaman's definition of norms as the high expectancy that illegitimate means are required to reach a.given goal. Actually, Dean's definition of the purposelessness component seems to approximate What Seeman would define as mean- inglessness, i.e., ”the low expectancy that satisfactory predic- 60 tions about future outcomes of behavior can be made.” Dean's third component, social isolation, although theoretically similar to Seeman's, is cperationalised quite differently. Whereas Seeman considers social isolation within the framework of rewards, Dean discusses it in terms of the number of social contacts that one has, and.the sense of separation from.groups'which is conse- quently engendered. Generally, it would seem that Dean's consider— stian of elie than is e te utilize the ‘ Because h' cynical re little to a: sequences 0 “in tetra ”mute m it Iould a use" to creating I 343m mile: ‘ ”Rural Ham dc 27 ation of alienation is somewhat similar to Seeman's, although there is a terminological difference in the way that they utilise the various terms. Because his study was primarily aimed at ascertaining empirical relations between sub-types of alienation, Dean has little to say in terms of the antecedent conditions and the con- sequences of alienation. Given his hypotheses regarding relation- ships between social status, age and urban residence, and his comments regarding his study paralleling those of wondell Bell, it would seem that he implicitly believes that differential access to goals, or social status is an important variable creating alienation. ‘ EaJda's conceptualisation of alienation is to some degree similar to Nettler's notion regarding the isolation from the cultural mainstream, and Dean's notion of social isolation.6‘ Hajda defines alienation as ”an individual's feeling of uneasi- ness or discomfort which reflects his exclusion or self-exclusion from social and cultural participation. It is an expression of non-belonging or non-sharing, an uneasy awareness or perception of 62 Eadda further conceives of unwelcome contrast‘with others.” alienation—-as Srole does of anomia-as a continuum that cannot be understood apart from the opposite feeling of ”belongingness.” Given this continuum, then, everyone in a society may at some situation or another, feel more or less alienated. Regarding the conditions under which alienation varies, Hajda conceives offbur major factors: [1) The numb! In individm‘. he participa in these col you out of (3} the deg: nezberstip g of I: commit: ties; (4) tj ‘0 Ihich on of the cool norms, beli 11th0ugh ‘ faich Rafi, tion, he '0\ the devolop Circ‘mnuc Ilium, 10!“; ‘ ‘3! Putic lliena‘ed he 1a: with ,9 ‘ilsuen ““0118 1 m”? r with the ship be, 28 (1) The number of qualitatively different collectivities an individual belongs to and thus the number of subcultures he participates in; (2) the extent to which the membership in these collectivities is concentrically coordinated with, grows out of, or is supported by the personal primary groups; (3) the degree to which the ties to chronologically earlier membership groups are not discarded or attenuated in favor of a commitment to new and substantially different social ties; (4) the extent to which the membership collectivities to which one belongs represent or symbolize the main body of the society and are infused with the prevalent values, norms, beliefs.53 Although these are the general characteristics or conditions which Hajda would see as associated with, or producing aliena- tion, he would further maintain that there are many routes for the development of alienation, dependent upon the particular circumstances. With respect to the possible consequences of alienation, Hajda has little to say. Again, it would seem that the particular consequences would depend upon the particular alienated group. The last theoretical contributions to alienation theory which we shall consider are those of Waisanen.64 In these efforts Waisanen is explicitly formulating a rapprochement between the various branches of alienation and anomie theory, as well as theory from various scientific disciplines. Waisanen begins with the notion that alienation is a consequence of the relatiar- ship between the individual and the social system. He, then hypothesizes that both the individual and the social system have norms and goals. This, of course, raises the possibility of several different types of discrepancies between individual and social system, and between goals and norms. This logical proced- ure produces a revision of Merton's paradigm. Then, waisanen relhtos th‘ P0 theory of F091 for the Mrs or implicitly theorists. 1 Mon no“ uh tosothin to not fit 1 use."65 1 “Milieu 1 amp“ to To this O! u .5931 that "its: [131] boo: than“ I mi 3 581 lo Dorcei h ”PM: 29 relates the possibility of a discrepancy to the dissonance theory of Pastinger using it as a meta-hypothesis to account for the 'strain toward adaptations' which is so often absent or implicitly assumed in the work of previous alienation-anomie theorists. Basically, restinger's meta-hypothesis is: "If a person knows two things, for example, something about himself and something about the world in‘which he lives, which somehow do not fit together, we will speak.of this as cognitive,giggggr ‘gngg,”65 Further, "if these two things do fit together, the condition is consonance. Dissonance prompts behavior which attempts to reduce the dissonance."66 To this framework, laisanen has attached what might be thought of as ”general goals" of the individual; or consequential elements that ”accrue to the person as he participates in the system . . . [and] become part of his self system."67 These consequential elements are: (1) "Familiarity [which] represents knowledge of, and a belief in the rules of the social system; it . . . relates to perceived stability. It makes patterned behavior possible. It represents internalisation of norms, perception of appropriate- ness of roles, and ability to manipulate facilities.” (2)”Power [or] influence within the system. It related to perceived sig- nificance and productivity, and.represents a consequence of eye- temic evaluation of the productivity of the person. Power implies knowledge of the processes of the system, and the influencing of these processes.“ (3) "The operation of the social system also yields sentiment, or affective ties with other members of the ”$93368 In the potion and $131 elements I produce the co isolution to I clement: v11]. iht individua'. in not influe to nu the Fe diuomnco re Factor: '1 to the 0,131: both mum "1 adding“ 30 system."68 When the conditions of the relationship between the person and the social system are discrepant, these consequen- tial elements are not forthcoming, and Iaisanen would argue, produce the conditions of powerlessness, normlessness and social isolation to which Seeman refers. Although these alienative elements will be present in any condition of discrepancy between the individual and the social system, they, in and of themselves, do not influence the direction which adaptations will take—or to use the Pestinger terminology—they do not affect the mode of dissonance reduction attempts. Factors which do affect the form of the adaptation are similar to the original Mertonian paradigm. Waisanen's introduction of both personal and social system considerations, however, produces an additional convergency with Seeman, and (:loward.69 Cloward notes, and Seeman cites, the followings It is our view that the most significant step in the with- drawal cf sentiments supporting the legitimacy of conven- tional norms is the attribution of the cause of failure to the social order rather than to oneself . . .thther the failure blames the social order or himself is of central importance to the understanding of deviant ccnduct.70 If the individual sees the social system at fault, he will seek changes in it, and vice versa. Il‘his additional element in Iaisanen's reformulation clarifies the form of adaptations to, or consequences of, alienation which were to some degree unclari- fied in Seeman's thought. Additionally, it also specifies the conditions of the relationship between the individual and the social system which produce alienation—another factor which is not present in Seeman's theoretical develczments regarding alienation. leieenen'e In our picture of '1 sally, he eppeu ficetion of both at the ooneeque «Map. It 1. huh to some (1 New, or the 1. There 13 Manuel. It mum 00nd: In indium“ him “n” '1‘“ Other us mum“! eh 31 waisanen's main contribution, then, has not been to alter our picture of what the state of the alienated person is; basi- cally, he appears to agree with Seeman. Rather, it is a clari- fication of both the conditions under which alienation develops, and the consequences under which certain forms of adaptation develop. It is, also, an explicit statement of a meta-hypothesis ‘which.to some degree sustains the dynamics of the alienative process, or the strain toward adaptation. Summary of Alienation-Anemia Theory 1. There is an ever-present problem with terminology and semantics. It is generally accepted that anomie refers to a societal condition and that anomy, anomia and alienation refer to an individual state. Others consider alienation to be the indi- vidual reflection of societal anomie. This is not incongruent with other usages but merely an additional semantic issue which eventually should be resolved. 2. lost researchers using the term anomia study the anteced- ents and consequent conditions, with anomia as an intervening variable; an exception (and this will become clearer in the section dealing with empirical studies) 1. the usage of the Srole scale, which is frequently used in antecedent-anomia studies, as well as anomiapconsequence studies. Theorists using the term alienation often are concerned with the nature of alienation, rather than its specific causes and/or consequences, although more persons seemed to be concerned with consequences. 3. Host theorists*wou1d agree with the basic rationale of a discrepancy be‘ moment of when this is : It least, logi :eticel vritin 4. Although comic in gene Manny $011 ‘11:. it 1. th I"fireman: mien u tell homer, , 83! mmogical 1 tech Other, 1 3101.5.“ '0 '1 210" Ire bee: entrained. wad hardly In emu, relatiOnghip there is E ii echievementa . .‘e ‘- [A .1 3 “Mine u an“ 13:11 Dr ' 32 discrepancy between an individual's goals and his actual achievement of them as a cause of alienation/anomia. In cases where this is not explicitly stated, it would appear that it is, at least, logically consistent. Therefore, we can combine theo- retical writings regarding alienation and ancmia—anomy. 4. Although it stems from a single general source, alienation/ anomia is generally thought to have various facets or sub-types, generally following one or more of the Seeman variants. Addition- ally, it is thought that alienation-at least at a theoretical 1evel-—should be conceptually separated from personal disorgani— sation as‘well as other quasi-pathological phenomena. There is, however, a general expectation that the sub-types, as well as the pathological phenomena, will be found.empirica11y related to each other. a problem stemming from this sort of conceptualisap tion—-as we shall see later-is that many of the operationaliza- tions are basically similar, and empirically have been found.to be correlated. If they do, in fact, stem from a single source, we could hardly expect them to be otherwise. In summary, then, we can define alienation as a product of the relationship of the individual and the social system, in which there is a discrepancy between the individual's expectations and achievements. Further, this condition creates for the individual, feelings of a disturbing sort, which may embody powerlessness, normleesness, etc. Although there is a theoretical basis for separating the various forms of alienation, there must await empirical proof of separability. Review 0 retiomlizet Pclloving th Iihh', lost so] cent: us their core or lees :- 1119, end the: llintione, I! this b01118 net and b! “1101 Srcle'e or It“mum ( Wm om: the “Drag. l f" M83 and that no“ pe: m“: not b. the 'Ol‘ld 'i 'Thea. a“; It“ One of Manes! to inane“! Wine; 11111 in“citing he! ‘0 ere 33 Review of hpirical Alienation-Mamie Literature Omrgticgliggtions of Alienation-Anon“ Following the general definition of alienation as a 'state of mind', most scholars have tended to use ”attitude type" state- ments as their measure of alienation. Host of these statements more or less represent what a person who was alienated might feel like, and thereby endorse. In discussing these various operation- alisations, we will use Seeman's terminolog as our standard, this being necessary because of the divergent terms which are used by various writers. Srcle's original anomia scale contained the following five statements: (1) "There's little use writing to public officials because often they aren't really interested in the problems of the average man”; (2) ”Nowadays a person has to live pretty much for today and let tomorrow take care of itself”; (3) ”In spite of what some people say, the lot of the average man is getting worse, not better"; (4) ”It’s hardly fair to bring children into the world with the way things look fcr the future”; and (5) "These days a person doesn't really know whom he can count on."71 Item one would seem to exemplify Seeman's ccnceptualisaticn of powerlessness, in that it indicates an inability of the person to influence what will happen to him, in the sense that he cannot influence that which his leaders will do. Item two, by indicating that "tomorrow” is essentially unpredictable, would seem to exemplify Seeman's conceptualization of meaninglessness, i.e., predict: lede. tee: f< although cone‘ em to be th. of the leek o: tural goals. fection with a hex-ion: g ihiimee g 5 Indeed, it he ”MN!" loz- lettlu'. ee en Mp1. 1“ I‘Oflecte , M n” “101': ”‘1‘“ 3°31: 3° you D" You '91‘. ’0 3° am You? D° you 01‘ 1 These ,0”. Glut: 1 Marlena. hticipfit‘ re; a. e reel: 34 i.e., predictions about future outcomes of behavior cannot be made. Item four is of much the same character. Item five, although construed by Srcle to mean social estrangement, would seem to be the social isolation discussed by Dean, in terms of the lack.of friendships, rather than estrangement from cul- tural goals. Item three was meant by Srcle to indicate dissatis- faction with the American progress ideal, and thereby rejection of an American goal, or, cultural estrangement. Rut item three also indicates a great deal of despair, or pessimism for the future. Indeed, it has been opined that the entire Srcle scale measures "despair” more than it does alienation or anemia.72 lettler's operationalisation of alienation is used by Seaman as an example of the 'isolation' component of alienation, i.e., it reflects a dissatisfaction with, and rejection of, the values of the majority society. Representative examples from Nettler's 11—item scale arse Do you enjoy TV? Do you read Reader's M? Iere you interested in the recent national elections? Do ;::;onal spectator-sports (football, baseball) interest Do you believe human life is an expression of devine purpose, or is it only the result of chance and evolution? 3 These sorts of items would appear to cperationalise a more or less 'intellectual alienation'. Clark, in cperationalising alienation (or more specifically, powerlessness) from a specific social system which an individual participates in, asked a series of questions about the individ- ual's feeling of influence within the social system. Specifically he asked who 0 :11 felt he he felt thet seal which he felt lutly, e ratz' thuhumm me to indie: til the indir °°°P0retive, 1 lie individua 1*‘3 has 0 “l in ldiiti Willem." Mien: 1. There in: 20 '.u 3° The 1 4. There 35 he askednwhc owned the cooperative, how much influence the individ- ual felt he had in the cooperative, how much ’say' the individual felt that members ought to have in the cooperative, the extent to ‘which he felt that he was a 'part-owner' of the cooperative, and lastly, a rating by the interviewer of the identification that the individual has with the cooperative.74 These items would seem to indicate powerlessness in the Seaman sense, in that they ask the individual how much influence and power he has over the cooperative, which in turn has some influence over the life of the individual. Dean has operationalised alienation as a general measure, and in addition, includes three sub-scales of powerlessness, normleesness and social isolation. Dean's items aree Powgrlegsness 1. There is little or nothing I can do towards preventing a major 'shocting' war. 2. we are just so many cogs in the machinery of life. 3. The future looks very dismal. 4. There is little chance for promotion on the job unless a.man gets a break. 5. There are so many decisions that have to be made today that sometimes I could just blow up. 6. It is frightening'to be responsible for the development of a little child. 7. Sometimes I have the feeling that other people are using me. 8. Ie're so reghmented today that there's not much room for choice even in personal matters. lormlgssness 1. I often wonder what the meaning of life really is. 2. Peoples' ideas change so much that I‘wonder if'we'll ever have anything to depend on. 3. Everything is relative and there just aren't any definite rules to this life. 4. lith so many religious beliefs today one doesn't really know which to believe. 5. I‘worry about the future facing today's children. —. O W‘s-Lyn) O O O .n‘ngfl H5??? (:3 «30‘ . o 36 6. The end often justifies the means. 7. The only thing one can be sure of today is that he can be sure of nothing. Socill Isolation 1. Sometimes I feel all alone in the world. 2. Real friends are as easy as ever to find. 3. People are just naturally friendly and helpful. 4. There are few dependable ties between people anymore. 5. I don't get invited out by friends as often as I'd really like. 6. lost people today seldom feel lonely. 7. One can always find friends if he shows himself n1.nd1ye 8. The world in which we live is basically a friendly pltOO e 75 Inspecting these items, it becomes apparent that Dean is treating social isolation, not as Seeman did (as rejection of culturally valued goals), but more as the perception of loneli— ness of the lack of friends. This is similar to Srcle's item, ”These days a person doesn't really know whom he can count on.” Dean's notions of normleesness also seem to approximate what Seeman.defined as meaningless, i.e., low expectancy of predict- ing the future, with one exception (item number six) where he states that ”The end often justifies the means”, which seems to exemplify approval of illegitimate means to get ahead. In addition, one must note in both powerlessness snd.normlessness, Dean emphasises the degree to which the future is either dismal or unpredictable. The two items concerning the future for children (powerlessnesse item six, normleesnesse item five) are similar to Srcle's item regarding the dismal future of children in today's world. There'would, however, appear to be a good deal of convergence between Dean's and Seaman's conceptualise- tion end cpl lejde'e . Clerk in th turticule cccnpetlcnl feeling of tetion, or dents “hoe Meence c religion, field the testes, , M011 fe ‘litnete: lam“, notion, °n° ie a loci”, 0m th Seeman. this ii See: mien: the I: ecgle 37 tion and cperationalization of powerlessness. Rajda's operationalisations of alienation are similar to Clark in that he was attempting to measure alienation*within a particular occupational sphere, in this case the academic occupations.76 Following his definition of alienation as a feeling of exclusion or self-exclusion from cultural partici- pation, or unwelcome contrast with others, Hajda asked respon- dents "how often they felt uncomfortably different in the presence of non-academic people because of their views on religion, views on politics, great interest in a specialised field that non-academic people do not understand, personal tastes, and concern about solving social problems.”77 If a person felt uncomfortable for these items, he was said to be alienated. This operationalisation is similar to that of Nettler, and would seem to represent a variant of Seaman‘s notions of isolation, in that there is a concern with what one is doing that is not in accord with the majority of the society. Hajda, however, includes 'uneasiness' or discomfort over this feeling as a factor in alienation. lettler and Seeman, however, would not apparently think it necessary for this discomfort to exist in order for alienation to exist. Seeman, after his definitive, ”On the Meaning of Alienation", undertook a series of other studies in which he operationalised the 'powerlessness' component of alienation.78 Although his scale has not been published in its entirety, nor has the writer been able to obtain one, Seeman does provide us with four emplee eh forced choi departure f Seaman's it 38 examples which he claims to be representative. They are forced choice types and, as such, represent something of a departure from the usual mode of asking attitude questions. Seeman's items aree 1. (a) There's very little persons like myself can do to improve world opinion of the United States. (b) I think each of us can do a great deal to improve world opinion of the United States. 2. (a) Persons like myself have little chance of protect- ing our personal interests when they conflict with those of strong pressure groups. (b) I feel that we have adequate ways of coping with pressure groups. 3. (a) Even if the odds are against you, it's possible to come out on top by keeping at it. (b) A person's future is largely a matter of what fate has in store for him. 4. (a) Nowadays people just don't realise what an important role luck plays in their lives. (b) There is really no such thing as luck. 5. (a) Many times I have the feeling that I have little influence over the things that happen to me. (b) I do not believe that chance and luck are very important in my life. 6. (a) Becoming a success is a matter of hard work; luck has little or nothing to do with it. (b) Getting a job depends mainly upon being at the right place at the right time. 7. (a) The average citisen can have an influence on the ‘way the government is run. (b) This world is run by the few people in power, and there is not much the little guy can do about it.79 Seaman's original definition of powerlessness was in terms of the ”expectancy of probability held by the individual that his own behavior cannot determine the occurrence of the outcomes, or reinforcements, he seeke."80 In his operationalisations, however, Seeman does not ask the person for this simplistic probability or expectation. Rather, he follows the usual practice of de egreed or dish etch e la! pro euro! tefinit inspection of stiller to De Retention of “36 from the blance to sro public offici lililetcn.‘ Wenonah. 39 practice of developing attitude type statements, which when agreed or disagreed‘with,‘would characterize a person who hold such a low probability. Seeman, thus, dilutes his relatively narrow definition of alienation at the operational level. An inspection of these items further reveals that they are somewhat similar to Dean's items, especially his items regarding the prevention of a 'shootingwwar', and promotion on the job stem— ming from the 'breaks'. Seeman's items also bear some resem- blance to Srcle's item regarding the uselessness of writing to public officials. Middleton, as a result of a survey of recent alienation-anomia operationalisations, has attempted to develop attitude statements 81 which will elicit all current usages of alienation. Middleton's items aree Powerlgssness There is not much that I can do about most of the important problems that*we face today. Meaninglessness Things have become so complicated in the world today that I really don't understand just what is going on. Hormlessness In order to get ahead in the world today, you are forced to do some things which are not right. Cultural Estranggment I am not much interested in the TV programs, movies, or magazines that most people seem to like. Soc i=1 lstrggemggt I often feel lonely. lstrgggement from work I don't really enjoy most of the‘work that I do, but I feel that I must do it in order to have other things that I need and'want.82 Middleton's notions of powerlessness and meaninglessness seem to parallel those of Seeman, and his notion of meaninglessness is very einll etetenent tion of m neceeeery to Seeman elitetior ccntenpo: lildlete mlgee, "Id Srol develop, "one t. new, 3‘“ Onl 1n 1 ten-1n 1Ovel, "We tital; by e. r°tli 11311 he“. cut, 1*. ‘ p13: 40 very similar to Dean's notions of 'normlessness'. Middleton's statement for normleesness explicitly follows Seeman's descrip- tion of normleesness as the expectancy that unapproved means are necessary to achieve a goal. Cultural estrangement is similar to Seeman's definition of 'isclation', and Nettler's operation- alisation of alienation, in that it represents the rejection of contemporary mass society values. Social estrangement, as Middleton lists it, is not included in the Seeman survey of usages, but approximates Dean's usage of social isolation, and Srcle's item about lack of friendship. Finally, Middleton develops an item which elicits estrangement from work, and seems to be very similar to Seeman's notion of 'self-estrange— ment', i.e., the feeling that behavior has no intrinsic value, but only in its effect or influence in obtaining future rewards. In summary, it'would appear that there is some amount of terminological confusion and empirical overlap at the operational level, as‘well as at the theoretical level. In spite of this overlap and confusion, however, there appear to be some rela- tively clear-out usages. Powerlessness, for instance, is seen by several writers (Dean, Seeman, Clark.and‘niddleton) as the feeling of the individual that he cannot control, or has no influence over, the majority of things that affect his life. Isaninglessness, in the Seeman sense, is somewhat less clear- out, both in terminology and empirical referents. Generally, it wound appear that meaninglessness refers to the inability to predict the outcome of any behavior. As such, the future appears as also voulc' and tom} There I referring nluee; a 10:1: of s is presen the Srcle let. Soc ”resent l‘l aliens Street. 39-311 tively 8‘ “d 1301. ‘ genera 41 appears as chaotic and dismal. This meaninglessness component also would appear to be present to some degree in Srcle's second and fourth items. There appear to be two clear varieties of isolation: one referring to the estrangement from, and rejection of, popular values; and the other referring to exclusion from a social net- ‘wcrk.of social ties, e.g., friendships, etc. This latter variant is present in Dean's social isolation scale, and in one item in the Srole scale, as'well as the intentional item in the Middleton set. Social isolation, as estrangement from mass values, is represented in Nettler's and Hajda's treatments, and is more of an alienation developed by intellectuals than by the man on the street. Be-examining Srole's scale, it would appear that it is a rela- tively general scale, containing powerlessness, meaninglessness and isolation from social ties, plus some content referring to a general pessimistic outlook for the future, e.g., despair. Dean's scales also contain these sorts of items, as well as some items about the future. As such, these two scales‘would seem to constitute the most general scales available in contemporary “...-8. e later-Correlations of Alienation Scale! 411though there is some basis for considering alienation as a separate but related phenomena, some sort of empirical tests are necessary. These are partially available, and an inspection of them may be helpful. First, from the Middleton study, it would 3?? afiVAwMAme - § Y‘ h‘ In. 42 appear that all items are relatively highly inter—correlated, with the exception of cultural estrangement (Table 1). Table 1a Inter—Correlations of Types of Alienation* 83 Meaning Norm Cult. Social Est. Estrange. Est. from work Powerlessness .58 .61 .06 .54 .57 Meaninglessness - .59 .17 .46 .81 Normlessness - - .31 .48 .67 Cultural estrangement ‘- - - .08 .20 Social estrangement - "- — e71 * The number of cases is 256; the measure of association is rule's Q. The values of 12 for all relationships for which Q exceeds .30 are significant at the .05 level. This would appear to give some justification for considering cultural estrangement as a separate, independent entity. From a variety of other studies, we can see other types of inter-correlations between scales. Table 2 is constructed from the studies of Dean, Nettler and Simmons.84 All figures are product moment correlations, and are significant at the .05 level. ‘Where no information is available, ”n.aF is entered. Table 20 Inter-Correlations of various Alienation.Measures Icwerlessness Normlessness Social Nettler Isolation Srcle Scale .35(s) .25 s .23(s) 3o1(n) .31 D Powerlessness .43 S .53 S n.a. .67 D .54 D Hormlessness .33 S n.a. .41 D Social Isolation n.a. Nettler D I Dean S - Simmons N I Nettler These if is some deg ationeliza — none of th cm the . l 8 3'0 recen‘ o “ there is —— ~_ utions o 7“ I ’8. 8 8' anHem 118 Pulp: (1) , (2) norm of 1 Eli: dete - Down '1‘}; g: Nihic “@7910 extra: .— - a _ V Ortho‘ 43 These inter—correlations would seem to indicate that there is some degree of empirical overlap between the various oper— ationalisations of alienation, and its sub-types. The fact that none of the correlations is very high, however, would also indi- cate that there is considerable room for independent variance. Two recent attempts to discover—-via factor analysis-dwhether there is a common factor underlying these various operationali- sations of alienation, do little to resolve our problem. Neal and Bettig, in their factor analytic study, presented the follow- ing purposes: (1; To develop.measures of pmwerlessness and normleesness; 2 to test the orthogonality of the powerlessness and normlessness measures from Srcle's anomie scale by means of factor analysis; (3) to compare the structure of alien- ation among'manual and nonpmanual workers; and (4) to determine the relationship of status aspiration to the powerlessness and normleesness dimensions of alienation. 85 lith these intentions, then, a series of items which measured political and economic powerlessness and normleesness were developed. Then, separate factor analyses‘were performed for manual and non-manual workers separately, with nine factors extracted. As expected, Srcle's anomie scale was found to be orthogonal to alienation, and various forms of alienation were found to be orthogonal to each other. Struening and Richardson, in another factor analytic attempt to determine the structure of the alienation-anomia-authoritar- ianism nexus, found that the Srcle scale fell upon the general dimension of 'alienation"which the alienation items also loaded on.86 Their conclusion‘was that alienation and anomia (Srcle) were of the complicate m Heel end Bet Provides no nelity of u M1 end 381 oblique fac‘ Therefore, 1 Muted. Sine. th “3 more :3 there has 1 em“. I: “1 twig. tlimatmn 44 were of the same general underlying factor. And to further complicate matters, Cartwright has written a critique of the Neal and Bettig article, stating that their factor analysis provides no answers to the questions of the possible orthogo— nality of the various factors.87 His principle reason is that Neal and Rettig's analysis did not allow for the possibility of oblique factors, and necessarily developed orthogonal factors. Therefore, he argues, their hypotheses are neither confirmed nor rejected. Rel tionshi s of Alienation to Other Variables Since the development of the Srcle scale for measuring anomia, and.more recently, the development of various alienation scales, there has been an explosion of empirical studies using these scales. In this section, we will review these studies, although our review cannot be exhaustive. Following our concern with alienation as a state of explicitly operationalised feeling of the individual, we will concern ourselves only with those studies using an attitudinal type of alienation or anemia. Most of these studies indicate a relationship between alienation and a more or less 'pathological' variable. By pursuing this review, we would also hope to note that alienation is a relatively stable area of phenomena (however it is operationalised) in that we hcpe to indicate that it is related consistently to a set of antecedent structural variables (usually social class) and to a set of consequential variables- usually held to be of a quasi-pathological nature. ‘we shall 04—” o-owm . conclude between One < beween piece, i mini tionehi on the in to 45 conclude with a discussion of anomia as an intervening variable between social class and these quasi-pathological variables. One of the most consistent findings has been a relationship between anomia/alienation and authoritarianism. In his original piece, Srcle reports a relationship between anomia and authori- tarianism, and prejudice, with anemia accounting for the rela- tionship of authoritarianism to prejudice.88 Roberts and Rokeach, on the other hand, contend that the relationship of authoritarian- ism to predudioe did not disappear when anomia was controlled for, but remained relatively high.89 Hobill, in a factor analyé sis, finds about the same thing, i.e., that both anomia and 90 authoritarianism contribute equally to prejudice. Dean also reports relationships between alienation (powerlessness, norm- lessness and social isolation sub-scales) and authoritarianism.91 Throughout this set of studies, it is clear that there does exist a relationship between alienation/anomia and authoritarianism-— and prejudice towards ethnic minorities. Other studies indicate that alienation/anomia is further related to other forms of pathological behaviors or attitudes. Simmons finds that anomia (Srcle scale) and alienation (Dean scales) are related to misanthropy, low self-esteem, life dis- satisfaction and attitude uncertainity.92 Rosenberg found that alienation-as operationalised by faith in people-is related to a willingness to use disapproved means in order to reach a goal.93 Angell finds a relationship between anomia and a‘will- ingness to invade other's privacy, as well as prejudice.94 45 McPhail finds a relationship between alienation-—as measured by the Dean scales—-and dogmatism.95 Rhodes finds that anomia is related to one's level of aspiration, regardless of the socio—econcmic level, with high anomia related to high aspire- tions (discrepancy between occupational aspiration and father's occupation)?6 Tumin and.Collins find a relationship between low anemia and readiness to desegregate, although Photiadis and Riggar found no relationship>of anomia to ethnic distance.9?’98 McDill and Ridley found that anemia and political alienation ‘were inversely related to voting, and having an opinion on political issues.99 Pearlin found that alienation from work 'was related to isolated*working conditions, feelings of limited achievement, dissatisfaction'with work rewards, and lack of social ties with co-workers.100 lettler and Rajda find that alienation (as cultural estrange— ment) is related to feelings of psychological instability‘lm’flm Seeman, in his various studies, has found that the powerlessness dimension of alienation is related to (1) learning new informa- tion about a social system, (2) organisational participation, (3) possession of objective knowledge about a social system, and satisfaction with this knowledge.103 Perhaps the most significant and stable finding, however, is the relationship between social status and anemia. This finds ing has recurred numerous times, and with many different measures of both anomia or alienation, and socio-economic status. In the empirical inventory of anemia studies provided in Clinard's hooie and 1 teen etudie: there tee a; These etl lover clue and to the the reletio f" lover o 47 Anemia d Deviant Behavior, we note that in all of the thir- teen studies explicitly involving status and anomia/alienation, there was an inverse relationship.104 These studies indeed lend credence to the notion that the lower class contains disproportionate numbers of anomie persons, and to the use of anomia as an interpretive variable explaining the relationship of lower classness and deviant behavior. That the lower class contains disproportionate numbers and examples of social problems is too well known to require documentation. m; In summary, it would appear that as yet anomia, alienation and the various sub—types of alienation are not highly inter- related, nor do they form an empirical identity. It may be that they are related to each other as are the different species of the same genus, as opposed to species of different genera. That is, at one level, using one set of comparisons, they are very similar, but comparing them to some other phenomena, they appear to be very different. All one could argue, at this point, is that they should be kept separate in the study of the relationship of alienation to variables of different genera. If they are similarly related to this variable, then it is obviously a logical difference between sub-types that makes no empirical difference (at least for that variable). 0f the various types of alienation, it would appear from both an empirical and operational point of view that the Seeman variant of isolation-—or cultural estrangement of Middleton, or 48 alienation of Nettler and Rajde—-is perhaps the most separable and distinct element. Isolation or cultural estrangement, however, is thought to be most characteristic of intellectuals, and, therefore, perhaps should not be considered a phenomena characteristic of mass society in general. If we recall Feuer's comments, it also may be that this cultural estrangement factor is the 'alienation' he is attributing to intellectuals. At this point, we shall also begin to take into account the characterizations of Rettler, Meier and Bell, and.McDill, re— garding the nature of the Srcle scale, in particular.105 Nettler feels that the Srcle scale does not measure anomia, but its correlate, despair. 'Meier and Bell concur, to a degree, and state that: ‘Ie are convinced that these questions for the most part measure despair, that is, utter hopelessness and discour- agement . . . We emphasise the notion of despair in the interpretation of our findings, although alienation appears to be measured in some degree as well . . .‘We have adapted the term 'anomia' to refer to the Srcle scale, but other terms such as 'despair', 'hopelessness', ' discouragement' , ' personal disorganization' , ' demorali- sation' (especially in the sense of disheartenment) in our understanding of the phenomena being measured.165 MoDill, upon finding that the Srcle scale statements, authori- tarianism, and prejudice, as well as economic factors loaded on one factor in a factor analytic study, characterised this under- lying dimension as: A Ieltanschamg which is negative in nature, that is, it represents a dim world view. The high loadings of all five of the anomie items reflect . . . a lack of inter- personal integration. Stated in other terms, this is the perspectivebof being'mastered by threatening forces beyond one's personal control . . . that the way to live and be supported in this threatening'world is through obedience to author or : rigi- Perhepe I': city of the c' simply the d: Semen implie clear that t] llienation. individuu i' ‘59 Various ‘1” or anon: nation. in lefinition c m“ by his the ”1.303“ 'it‘miou. 61"“ the mud “The; tiOn, .113. I‘ 49 to authority figures and through conventionalism or a rigid acceptance of the status quo. 107 Perhaps‘what is at issue here is the narrowness or specifi- city of the definition of alienation. If anomia/alienation is simply the discrepancy between desired and achieved goals (or as Seeman implies, a low expectancy of control, etc.), then it is clear that the Srcle scale, as well as others, do not measure alienation. 0n the other hand, if anomia is the state of the individual in such a situation, then that which is measured by the various scales may also logically be thought of as aliena— tion or anomia. It is instructive to note Seemsn's operational- isations in this regard. Although he gives a relatively narrow definition of alienation, his operational definitions-as indi- cated by his scale statements-are relatively broad, and include the personal feelings of the individual in the low expectancy situation. Given the stable, but relatively low inter-correlations, we 'weuld perhaps argue that 'despair' may be a sub-type of aliens- tion, and subject to the same family-genus classification we specified on the previous page. Impirically, we note a high correlation between class and anomia, and between anomia and social problems. To this point, we can conclude that anomia,may”well be an intervening variable between class and social problems. This sets the stage for a consideration of other types of stratification variables which are also related to social problems, and which may exemplify the Durkheimélerton rationale and thereby create anomia. Sc It has bee Iteble phenoo nd queei-pet erplein these ing the deveI lover-class 1 tell: he ehox '0 'ould eeel of etratifice 5°“ m eoc Mum the reason; for n that son“ a logical, or nu 11‘! end 50 Social Mobility and Status Inconsistency It has been indicated that anomia and alienation are relatively stable phenomena in terms of their relationship to lower classness and quasi-pathological behaviors. The rationale utilized to explain these findings is the basic DurkheiméMerton theory regard- ing the development of a discrepancy between those goals that a lower-class person is able to achieve, and those goals which he fells he should legitimately be able to attain. At this point, we would seek to extend this rationale to include two other types of stratification variables which logically may also produce anomia. These are social mobility and status inconsistency. This section presents the argument that there are both theoretical and empirical reasons for making this extension. First, it may be illustrated that social mobility and status inconsistency theoretically and logically exemplify the DurkheiméMerton rationale; second, social mobility and.status inconsistency are empirically related to a number of quasi-pathological variables of the same sort that anomia and lower classness are. Therefore, it would be congru- ent to hypothesise a relationship between social mobility and status inconsistency, and alienation. Social Mobility In Suicide, Durkheim explicitly singles out mobility as a factor in creating conditions which in turn lead to anomia. In downward.mobility, for example: They [mobile persons] must reduce their requirements . . . their moral education has recommenced. But society cannot adjust them instantaneously to this new life and teach them to practice the increased self-repression to which they are oonditior intoleral That is, per: expectations but attain t1 tiecrepancy 1 the em Gym It“use I p6: l‘gitinately Person to do ‘1“ notes t ation. is re on""‘ripping lad “Skin, 5 he 'hOUld so In Wotan “11:1“, as 51 they are unaccustomed. So they are not adjusted to the condition forced upon them, and its very prospect is intolerable. 108 That is, persons who are downwardly mobile continue to base their expectations for goal achievement upon their previous statuses, but attain these goals with their new lowered status, so that a discrepancy exists, which in turn creates anomia. Approximately the same dynamic applies to upward mobility. In hie new higher status, a person attains more goals than he could have expected legitimately in hie old status. This, in turn, leads the mobile person to doubt the efficacy and.legitimacy of all norms. Durkheim also notes that when the efficacy of the norms as a limit to aspir— ations is reduced, the aspirations of the individual spiral upward, outstripping the attainments that even the new status can afford. And again, a discrepancy exists between what the individual thinks he should achieve, and what he actually achieves. In contesporary sociology, this rationale has been little modified, as is evident by inspecting Lipset's recent survey of social mobility and its consequences: Perhaps the most important key to an explanation of such varying consequences of mobility . . . is the concept of status discrepancies . . . The few analyses of the psycho— logical dimension of this problem that have been made indicate that status discrepancies may cause difficulties in personal adjustment because of high self-evaluations 109 in one sphere of life conflict with low ones in another. Lipset then goes on to cite Durkheim and his original rationale for the relationship of mobility and anemia, presumably indicating that .Durkheim's interpretation remains adequate. This general rationale of strain is used to explain the rela- tionships of mobility to mental illness,110 prejudice,111 social isolation, 11 or radicalis pathologies] for example quonoos of 1 thhoug‘n Waging cc rationale e ”mines t] per so. '1‘ 1' far fro mobile '91 for highs anomie . 52 political apathy,113 or political liberalism 114 isolation,112 or radicalism, etc. Lipset also cites as evidence of the pathological consequences, ”many recent books (The Exurbanites, for example)[which] portray the damaging psychological conse- quences of mobility and competition within elite professions.115 Although fairly numerous studies indicate the possible damaging consequences of mobility, and utilise the discrepancy rationale as an explanatory device, only one study explicitly examines the relationship between social mobility and anemia, per so. This was done by.Meier and Bell, and the relationship 116 Among those of low status: the upwardly is far from clear. mobile were less anomie than the stable and downwardly mobile; for higher status persons, however, the stable were the least anomie. This would seem to indicate some degree of interaction or contamination of social mobility with average status. ‘we shall return to the relationship of status inconsistency, social mobil- ity and average status in a later section. Status Inconsistency Status inconsistency had its beginning*with Hughes' concern over status as position, and Benoit-Smullyan's concern with various types of status.117 Hughes argues that a position came to have associated with it, certain non-essential or secondary, yet 'oharaoteristic' factors, e.g., the position ”doctor" has attached.to it the characteristics of also being male, white and somewhat older. Hughes was concerned, then, with persons fitting the primary characteristic of a role but not the secondary char- ecteristil He srgoes situation asteristi: ioctor vit eet, then sedation Benoit 'hich one 810ml Phi 31mph, . ”Naive . then take 53 acteristics, e.g., the doctor who is young, Negro and female. He argues that such persons were often placed in ambiguous situations because others had come to expect the secondary char- acteristics as well as the primary characteristics, and the Negro doctor violated these expectations. Given this ambiguous status set, then, the person may be forced to make some sorts of accom- modations to this unusual situation. Bencit-Smullyan, however,‘was concerned.with the degree to which one conceives of status as a unidimensional or multi-dimenp sional phenomenae Many persons, like Hollingshead and‘larner, for example, utilise a uni-dimensional conceptualization.118 They conceive of social status as a single underlying variable, and then take a number of variables as indicators of positions on this unidimensional continuum. This set of indicators is then 'weighted, etc. (or more likely, averaged) so as to obtain the best possible index of the person's standing on the status or social class continuum. Others, however, conceive of status as being composed of several different variables, and not necessarily unidimensional, or as indicative or average standing on a general continuum. Given this possibility, then, there also exists the possibility what a person's scores on the set of variables may or may not be congruent, i.e., there may be more or less vari- ance in the individual status set. It‘was not until 1954 that the possibility of a multi-dimen- sional view of social class was acted upon, by Lenski, in his original paper, "Status Crystallization: A.lon-Vertical Dimen- lion of 5 way of a an income subject t 1150:! eta tho had c and findi Isnski .u example 0 different Iould be 1 human; “‘8an of Uthow “My, 11. thill a" ‘ tency 'qm ins th. ,1 54 sion of Social Status."119 In this paper, there was little by ‘way of a priori theory, except to suggest that a person in such an inconsistent, or incongruent position, would in some way be subject to strain. Lenski argues that persons with uncrystal— lised statuses would be more liberal than would.those persons ‘who had congruent statuses. After testing this hypothesis, and finding a relationship between inconsistency and liberalism, Lenski suggested the 93.1139. explanatory devices citing Hughes' example of the Negro doctor, he argues that persons who occupy different positions on several different status hierarchies 'would be more likely to have experiences of an unpleasant and frustrating nature, which'wculd lead them to search for an avenue of escape, or rectification of this frustrating situation. Although there were several studies using status inconsis- tency, little else was done of a theoretical nature regarding this new variable. Sampson, however, places status inconsis- tency squarely in the Merton-Durkheim tradition, by emphasis- ing the expectations that accompany statuses, and arguing that when one occupies discrepant statuses, then one is also subject 12° Sampson discusses to contradictory expectations of others. ”expectations" in the interpersonal behavior context. It'would also appear logical to place it into the more general "goal at- tainment” context (i.e., cne's expectations of rewards are based upon the statuses that one holds). Then, he places this possi- bility of discrepancy into the psychological framework of restinger's dissonance theory, and.argues that status inconsis- tency alsc a etrain 1 eietent p1 called out Interee in resins: laieanen.‘ “1 tender link bets. Aside : be ”Phi! ui'nltio: 'hioh Pro mhologi "mt. s toms Of I"thblogi In Len 'hiP bety infirm.” 55 tency also produces cognitive dissonance, which in turn produces a strain towards reduction of the dissonance. Thus, the incon- sistent person is called upon to make efforts that would not be called out in a person who occupied consistent statuses. Interestingly enough, the convergence developed by Sampson is reminescent of the convergence in alienation theory by 121 By fm-ther placing these two theories into logi- Iaisanen. cal tandem, we are able to demonstrate a logical theoretical link between status inconsistency and alienation/anomia. Aside from theoretical reasons, there would also appear to be empirical reasons for suggesting such a relationship. If alienation is an intervening variable betwaen social conditions which produce discrepancies in expectations, and more or less pathological behaviors, then status inconsistency, Just as average status and social mobility, should be related to various forms of behaviors which can be construed as 'adaptive' or pathological. These sorts of relationships appear to exist. In Lenski's original study, for example, he finds a relation- ship between status inconsistency and political liberalism, and interprets political liberalism to be an effort at changing the nature of the social order so that discrepancies in status will not exist . 122 Further, Goffman finds a relationship between status inconsistency and expression of a desire for a change in the distribution of power, and Geschwender finds that incon- sistents are much more likely to participate in change-oriented acups.123’124 Ringer and Sills find that inconsistents are much more likely to be political extremists than consistents."25 In tor: Jackson ti symptoms c eistents a consistent eistents e linority 5 foreign bc utility; ”idhborhc inconsistq hunt or in81110 31 In tun Itatm 1m "Min bet “Wally < 'tltug“ ‘ “d mu: u th' be} hm‘dfi a Md. 1:: of “Pact, . “3°11 inteaatn 10°3er 3.} 56 In terms of more explicitly pathological types of behaviors, Jackson finds that inconsistents are more likely to exhibit symptoms of psychological stress, and Lenski finds that incon- sistents are more likely to be socially isolated, than are ocnsistents.126’127 Geschwender also has evidence that incon- sistents are more likely toe (1) express hostilities toward minority groups in the work force, e.g., women, Negroes and foreign born; (2) have higher geographical and horizontal Job mobility; (3) express less satisfaction with their Job and their neighborhood.“28 The author also had data which indicate that inconsistents rate themselves lower as to where they stand at present on a rating scale with the best possible life they can imagine at one end, and the worst possible life at the other."29 In emery, our argument states that social mobility and status inconsistency exemplify conditions which may produce a strain betwun that which an actor expects, and that which he actually obtains. Following Durkheim, we would argue that statuses are composed of sets of norms, which specify the rights and privileges or rewards associated with the status, as well as the behaviors which are required of such statuses. Further, knowledge of various statuses furnishes persons not only with a guide to their own behaviors and expectations, but with a set of expectations for other persons in particular statuses. Now, in a society with a large degree of specialisation and loose integ-ation, it is possible for various statuses also to be loosely articulated, so that at any one time, an individual may possess several different statuses. It is also possible for a person to de‘ any combinat: expectations statuses. If these actor is in Consider the hen a high If the POrso Oducatioml obtains r"; th“ ! diner 'm 119 actu it “541.181 57 person to develop expectations based upon any one status or any combination of these statuses, and have other persons make expectations of him, based upon any combination of these statuses. If these statuses are not supportive or congruent, then, the actor is in a position of conflicting demands and expectations. Consider the status inconsistency situation where a person may have a high educational status, and a low occupational status. If the person bases his expectation of rewards upon his high educational status and expects high rewards, and he actually obtains rewards or goals based upon his low occupational status, then a discrepancy exists between the reward he expected and what he actually obtained. This is not to argue that such an expectation-achievement discrepancy will, in fact, always develop. It is to argue, as did Horton with respect to the lower class, that such persons are more likely to develop such a discrepancy than are persons who do not possess such incongruent statuses. And to the degree that they are more likely to develop discrep- ancies, they are also more likely to develop alienation. such the same rationale exists for social mobility, in that the person develops expectations based on one status, and then moves away from that status into another, so that the possibil- ity of a discrepancy is more likely than if he had not been mobile, either upward or downward. Given the above discussion, it would appear Justifiable to hypothesize a relationship between social status, social mobil- ity, and status inconsistency and alienation, because they all exhibit the expectation to alienati tion of the then we mus betteen av: stratei. for the a: class and Prior ‘ 138110 of b. 1.180113 mm W In“, ‘lienati The “ti 1°16“ of “1311c“ tap 0f 1 mute. 58 exhibit the possibility of developing a discrepancy between expectations and attainments. If they are, in fact, related to alienation, this would constitute an extension and valida- tion of the Durkheim-Merton rationale; if they are not related, then‘we must seek some other explanation of the relationship between average status and anomia, which has been amply demon- strated. ‘lb must also, then, seek an alternative explanation for the cisting relationships between mobility, inconsistency, class and these more or less pathological characteristics. Prior to a simple statement of hypotheses, however, the issue of the inter-relationships of these three variables must be discussed, as must the nature of alienation. Relationship of Average Status, Social Mobility and Status Inconsistency Let us begin our discussion with the relationship of class to alienation, and more or less use it as our base of presentation. The rationale here is that there is within the cultural knowh ledge of the United States, a set of specified goals, which are applicable to all Americans. Then, as one gets closer to the top of the stratification system, one should achieve propor- tionately more of these culturally specified goals. And, con- sequently, there should be proportionately lees alienation, when one compares a higher status‘with a lower status. This would appear to be the case. The relationship of average status and social mobility with respect to alienation, however, is not independent. Consider umward.mobility, for instance. The very change itself is likely to create a c upmrdly sob: highs atatu he should he could 01min “Wing If mother he nobility pe “I adverse The inane ¢ Waons, t] i" in add higher the “'1? 10m in “nu; Person Ih 1113tu Wrath, hm. of 59 to create a discrepancy, and thereby create alienation. in upwardly mobile person, however, has also moved up, and has a higher status, so that when compared to lower-class persons, he should have $23: alienation than they. In any case, one could examine the effects of upward mobility, per so, only by comparing within one class, stable and upwardly mobile persons. Another factor mentioned by Meier and Bell, is that the upward mobility per se is a goal achievement which may in fact mitigate any adverse effects due to goals outstripping achievements.130 The issue of control, however, remains. For downwardly mobile persons, the reverse of the above argument should operate. That is, in addition to the mobility effects (which should produce higher than average alienation) we also have the effects of the newly lower status, as well as the ignominy of a recent decrease in status. In this situation, however, the downwardly mobile person should be more alienated than either the stable or the upwardly mobile person. If we accmulate the various factors operating in mobility, it would appear Justifiable to rank, in terms of the expected amount of alienation, mobile persons of m class in the following orders downwardly mobile, stable, and upwardly mobile. The downwardly mobile should be the most alien- ated because they have not only the newly acquired low status, but also because of the ignominious effects of 'failure'. Iith respect to the comparison of upwardly mobile and stable persons of the same class level, there appear to be three empir- ical alternatives, with three different theoretical interpreter- tions. First, if the upwardly mobiles are no more alienated than the stables it: equally the uplud: the stable dionated cm of so while per in stable tht upwu. Person, w not °°lmt loads up“ to the s' 60 the stables, then we would argue that the "success” of mobil- ity equally balances the effect of the change per so, so that the upwardly mobile person is no more or no less alienated than the stable person. Second, if the upwardly mobile person is‘;gg§ alienated than the stable person, we would argue that the suc- cess of mobility over—balances the effect of change, so that the mobile person has achieved 2.9.12.2 of his goals than the person who is stable in a class, and is thereby less alienated. Third, if the upwardly mobile person is more alienated than the stable person,'we would argue that the success of upward.mobility does not counterbalance the effect of change per so, so that this leads the mobile person to a state of high alienation compared to the stable person. Another confounding factor is the impossibility of a person in the highest class being downwardly mobile, at least in terms of intra-generational and likewise, it is impossible for a person in the lowest class to be upwardly mobile. This is an artifact of our mode of constructing the class index, but nevertheless, it is unavoidable-if one uses intra-generational mobility as his index of mobility. In order to obtain independence of mobility from class-to thereby assure us of the logical possi- bility of downwardly'mobile persons in the highest class levels (and upwardly mobile persons in the lowest class leve1)-dwe ‘weuld suggest the use of inter-generational rather than intra- generaticnal mobility. Inter-generational mobility also has its share of problems. In terms of the discrepancy created by mobility, and the amount o! tims an obvious the tional sobi tho differs inns-gens: vould intro intsr- u ' " 'ould st “1" logics h to tk mansion, 'Wltiom "lfiionah; ”Wisteria; flintion but [uggea 61 of time available for ‘adjustment' to the discrepancy, it is obvious that there is more 'adjustment time' in intergenera— tional mobility than in intro-generational mobility. Due to the differences in this adjustive potential, it is likely that inter-generational mobility will produce less alienation than would intra-generational mobility. In that Meier and Bell find inter- as well as intra-generational mobility related to anomia, we would still suggest that inter-generational mobility is a more logically adequate measure for this study.131 As to the theoretical and logical relationships of status inconsistency and social mobility, we can make few a priori suggestions. It would appear that there are logically possible relationships between almost any type of mobility and status in- consistency. But it depends largely upon the type and operation- alisation of mobility. We shall not delve deeply into this area but suggest a few of the problems involved, and the tenative resolution that we propose. First, there is the general problem of inter- versus intra- generational mobility. If we consider intros-generational mobil- ity, then status inconsistency and mobility may be confounded. hr example, a young college graduate, as yet non-mobile because he has his first lowly Job with low income, may be classified as inconsistent as his education is out of line with his Job and income. But if he is interviewed after his mobility, he may be classified as consistent. 0n the other hand, an older, high school graduate, who has been occupationally mobile into a highly rat the former it results ulstionst inconsistt in n ind Mittens his eye] bility 91 5'6?“ 01 lith j Perhap. : relatgd ‘ city, 1211 in°°nsis "4 ‘ ac 62 highly rated occupation, will be labeled as inconsistent. In the former case, mobility resulted in consistency; in the latter, it resulted in inconsistency. In any case, working out the relationships between intrapgenerational mobility and status inconsistency‘would.require an extensive history of the changes in an individual's education, occupation and income, with cal- culations of mobility and consistency at several stages of the life cycle. This would also introduce a problem in the compara- bility of life cycle stages and generations, as well as a high degree of complexity. Iith inter-generational mobility, the problem remains, but perhaps is not so extensive. Insofar as occupations are cor- related with other prestige factors, and especially with ethni- city, then the inter-generational mobility of a son may produce inconsistency. For example, if a father is a Negro laborer, and a son is occupationally mobile, but not ethnically mobile, he is mobile to an inconsistent position. Or, if a father is a white banker, then an occupational mobile (downward) son may move into an inconsistent situation. These are the only fore- seeable logical possibilities of spuriousness in the status inconsistency mobility nexus. Perhaps the most serious control problem stems from the fact that status inconsistency is not logically independent of status. It is logically impossible for persons who are in the highest and lowest average status levels to be inconsistent by virtue of the fact that to be in the lowest or highest class, individuals must have consistently high or low ranks on all dimensions. Average st; person is status is in only 1} meet th is wit tween; 1 °°nf01md increase, 311* also 1' more 63 Average status, in the middle status ranges, may occur if the person is consistently middle on all dimensions‘2£_if his middle status is a combination of both high and low statuses. And it is only in the exact middle of the status range that one can expect the highest degrees of inconsistency. As with mobility, then, the possibility exists that the simul- taneous effects of status inconsistency and average status will confound each other in these middle levels. That is, as one increases status he is decreasing the likelihood of alienation. But also, as one increases status, from low to middle levels, one is more likely (logically) to be inconsistent-or at least in a statistical-logical sense. This problem of the confounding effects of status inconsist— ency and average status has been recognised in previous studies, but these previous attempts at handling the problem have left much to be desired. Ienski and Landecker, for example, have approached control by removing from the 'extremely inconsistent' group those respondents with the most extreme status scores (both high and low status) until the inconsistent and.consist- ent levels had equal status averages.132’133 In effect, this ‘was a form of'matching. But only for the consistency level as a group. And, it is not a random form of matching, i.e., the high and low status persons'were not randomly selected, but were removed systematically. This, of course, takes out of the sample those persons who-—due to their extreme status positions- might contribute most to a relationship between average status and alienation. In View: between s' ity to al contain I “88s“ s to schis‘ of ststu 5.19 rang This, of t0 assm these t1 ”Slug 1 in alig mm“ Tm there a at1°21 . Bub-11 My (:0 to a“ 64 In view of the above, a shmple statement of a relationship between status inconsistency, average status, and social mobil- ity to alienation is not an adequate test; the hypothesis must contain within it a statement of control. Therefore, we would suggest a traditional form of multi-variant control. In order to achieve this, though, one must have a relatively large number of status groups or levels, so that there'will be several 'midp dle range' levels of status in which inconsistency may vary. This, of course, means that one must have a relatively large N to assure sufficient representation in all status levels. ‘lith these two conditions satisfied, it would be possible to hold status more or less constant, and to observe the differences in alienation among the various status inconsistency groups ‘within a given status level. ‘Iith respect to the tenative dependent variable, alienation, there are two suggestions. First, we have observed that alien- ation apparently has several related but partially independent sub-dimensions. Specifically, our observation was that they may constitute a unitary phenomena when placed in Juxtaposition to some variables, yet independent phenomena when counterposed to other variables. Therefore, our suggestion is that aliena— tion be considered in its sub-dimensions for any study, and that in that study a determination be made as to whether or not alienation constitutes separate or a single dimension. we must also recall the conclusions of‘McDill, and.Moier and Bell, regarding the nature of much of this alienation-anomia phenomena, leltenschdt event that dent seam sensors on Stratific: 11th t «rise of t° aliens 65 phenomena, i.e., that it measure 'despair' or a ”negative ‘leltanschauung” rather than alienation or anomia.134 In the event that this is so, we would suggest a logically indepen- dent measure of satisfaction with one's life, or despair. This measure can then be correlated.with alienation, and the various stratification measures. lith these qualifications, then, it is possible to state a series of hypotheses regarding the relationship of stratification to alienation. Hypotheses Our very general hypothesis is that if there are relationships between average status, status inconsistency and social mobility to alienation, then Durkheim-Merton discrepancy rationale is once more validated. If all of these relationships are not present, however, then a different explanatory rationale is necessary to handle the relationships which do in fact exist, particularly that of social class to alienation. Given the nature of our arguments thus far, however, a more specific detailed statement of hypotheses is deemed necessary. 1.‘Ie expect an inverse relationship between average status and the various forms of alienation, i.e., powerlessness, norm- lessness, social isolation, as well as to one's estimation of his present standing, and his future outlook. That is, we expect persons with lower average status to be more alienated than persons‘with higher average status. 2. so expect little or no relationship between status incon- sistsnoy and present star: 3. lo ex; nobility and Specificall: the nest a1: tstsesn Sta lllenatlon, 4. (s) I “11 81161131 nobility u (b) ‘ ‘li'nation statm me “Met the Ihen the ‘ “mm“ (c) 66 sistency and the various forms of alienation, estimation of present standing, and future outlook. 3.‘lb expect that there will be a relationship between social mobility and alienation, present standing, and future outlook. Specifically, we expect that downwardly mobile persons will be the most alienated, etc., and relatively little difference between stable and upwardly mobile persons, with respect to alienation, etc. 4. (a) we expect that the relationship between average status and alienation, etc., to remain constant when controls for social mobility and stat inconsistency are instituted. (b) so expect that a relationship of status inconsistency to alienation, etc., will become obvious when controls for average status and social mobility are instituted. Literally, we would expect that these relationships'will be pronounced and definite ‘when the controls are instituted, because of the removal of the contaminating effects of average status and social mobility. (c) we expect that the relationship of social mobility to alienation, etc., remain constant or to increase when controls for average status and status inconsistency are instituted. Especially for average status, where the relationship of status to alienation‘will be controlled, and the ”confounding” effects for upwardly mobile persons are removed, i.e., in this instance, ‘we would expect that there will be a discrimination between stable persons in a status level, and persons upwardly mobile to that status level,‘with the latter category having the higher level of alienation, etc. 5. (a) W to alieneti status incc simultaneoa (bl‘ sistency a ‘he relat'j Ihen simu “bility (c: it! and J tied (0v order 1s Biatenc; 67 5. (a) We expect that the relationship of average status to alienation, etc., will remain constant when controls for status inconsistency and social mobility are instituted simultaneously. (b) so expect that the relationship between status incon- sistency and alienation, etc., will be further intensified (over the relationship we expect to appear at the second order level) ‘when simultaneous controls for average status and social mobility are instituted. (o) so expect that the relationship between social mobil- ity and alienation, etc., will be further intensified and clari- fied (over the relationship‘we expect to appear at the second order level) when controls for average status and status incon- sistency are instituted simultaneously. Finally, we would note that the general hypothesis will be tested only by testing specific hypotheses 4 and 5; hypotheses 1, 2, and 3 will not provide such a test. Hypothesis 1 will (should it be confirmed) provide us with an estimate of valid— ity, i.e., if there is a relationship in this study between average status and alienation, we can assume that we are measuring the same sort of phenomena that has been measured in other studies reporting relationships between average status and alienation. 1. Emile Du George Sim] 2. kills D 300m Sin 3' M's " Mn 5. leg 5. Robert N" York: 7- his 3. m. 9e E“ FOOTNOTES — CHAPTER I 1. Emile Durkheim, The Division of Labor in Societ , trans. George Simpson (New York: The Free Press, 1947). 2. Emile Durkheim, Suicide, trans. John A. Spaulding and George Simpson (New York: The Free Press, 1951). 30.£E;Ee’ P. 241. 4. Ibid., p. 250. 5. Ibid. 6. Robert K. Merton, Social Theor and Social Structure (rev.ed.; New York: The Free EW— 7. gig" p. 162. 8. Egg. 9. 933., p. 133. 10. Ibid.: p. 162. 11. Why anomie should create disequilibrium and a consequent strain towards adaptation as a method of restoring equilibrium has never been fully discussed. 12o ”Orton, 22' 2&3}, Pe 162(fn.2) and. De 164(fne8)e 13. There has been a traditional terminological turmoil as to what term to use, e.g., anomy, anomia, anomie, etc. We shall use 'anomia' to refer to an individual condition and anomie to refer to a societal condition. 14. Robert Dubin, ”Deviant Behavior and Social Structures Continu- ities in Social Theory,” American Sociological Review, 24(April, 1959). 147-164. 15. Richard A. Cloward, ”Illegitimate Means, Anomie, and Deviant Behavior,” Angricgg Sociological Review, 24(April, 1959), 164- 176. 16. James 1'. Short, Jr., ”Gang Delinquency and Anomie,” Anomie and Deviant ahfvior, ed. Marshall B. Clinard (New rot-ks The Free Pr...’ 19 4 9 ppe 98-127e 17. Edwin ll. Lemert, ”Social Structure, Social Control and Devi- ation,"in Clinard’ 22. cite, pp. 57.97e 18. Among those who have advanced salient criticisms of anomie and anomia theory per so are Howard S. Becker, Alfred R. Lindesmith and John Gagnon. 19. We will not consider Lasswell here because of his explicitly psychological bent. 68 20. Robert bouillon C 21. Leo Sro to hplorat b”: 1956): 22' Mn 23. M” equates mo others, how “‘1 because 01' anomie) lbw”! 25' £13” I 26' MN I "“5103 of "V have has the! affect. s°°i°1° cat 28' Herbert anly’I 4 m 19 (its? 29. Lou. F! 3061010 Oi '°°d Cliffs 59 20. Robert M. MacIver, The Ram ts We Guard (New York: The Macmillan Co., 1950), pp. 84—92. 21. Leo Srcle, ”Social Integration and Certain Corollariess An Exploratory Study," American Sociological Review, 21(Decem- ber, 1956), 710. 22a Ibide, Pe 710e 23. Ibid., p. 711. It is evident from this quote that Srcle equates anomia with alienation. Because he cites Durkheim and others, however, and because he terms his scale an anomia scale, and because other persons using his scale place it in the anomia (or anomie) tradition, we have chosen to treat his work here. 24. Ibid., p. 712 (italics supplied). 25e Ibide, p. 716a 26. Ibid., p. 711. This follows rather closely Merton's 1957 revision of Socigl Theon and Social Structure. These revisions may have been available to Srcle, but one cannot know how much they affected his work. 27. Albert M. Cohen, ”The Sociolog cf the Deviant Act,” American Sociologicgl Review, 30(February, 1965), 5(fn.1). 28. Herbert McClosky and John H. Schaar, ”Psychological Dimensions of Anomy,” Am rican Sociolo cal Review, 30 (February, 1965), 14 and 19 (italics added). 29. Lewis Feuer, ”What is Alienation? The Career of a Concept,” Sociolggz on Trial, eds. Maurice Stein and Arthur Vidich (Ingle- 'wcod Cliffs, N.J.s Printice-Hall, Inc., 1963). 30. Ibid., p. 127. 31. £31., p. 128. 32. Ibid., p. 129. 33. Georg Simmel, "The Metropolis and Mental Life," The Sociology of G or Simmel, trans. Kurt H. Kolff (New York: The Free Press, 1950). 34. Feuer, 2p. gig” pp. 138, 146. 35. Gwynn Nettler, "A Measure of Alienation," American Sociologi- cal Ravi , 27 (December, 1957). 670-677. 360 we, Pa 672e 370 M03 P0 6740 38. Melvin Seeman, ”On the Meaning of Alienation,” American figsdolcgical Review, 24 (December, 1959), 783-791; Melvin Seeman and John W. Evans, ”Alienation and Learning in a Hospital Set- ting,” American Sociolo ical Review, 21 (December, 1962), 772- 782; Melvin Seeman, ”Alienation and Social learning in a Reform- atory,” Amggican Journgl of Sociology, 59 (November, 1963). 270- 2843 Arthur G. Neal and Melvin Seeman, ”Organisations and Power- lessnesss A Test of the Mediation Hypotheses," American Sociolo ical Seeman, "Ant 3 (hr-June, 39: 0f th. Salomon Rett some effilia 40o Seem, 41' MU P 42' Mu Pl 43' Mu P 44' M" P 45' £13.” P 45- Seem, ‘7' Mn I 48. M. ‘90 RattOrI' pr”listing 'hich 1" IOI 50' Freda-1. W 51. seem, “mien . 'Ould ”Elle mm in ‘ tion be 1301 to “86981: “tidote. 7O Sociological Review, 29 (April, 1964), 216-226; and Melvin Seeman, ”Antidote to Alienation-Learning to Belong," Trans-action, 3 (May-June, 1966), 35-40. 39. Of the various writers about alienation, Arthur Neal, Salomon Rettig, Melvin Seeman, John Clark, Dwight Dean have had some affiliation with Ohio State University. 40. Seeman, ”0n the Meaning of Alienation,” 22. 333., p. 783. 41- 22%." P0 784. 42. £81519, p. 786. 43. 2931., p. 788. 44. 32.42.. p. 789. 45. ELLE-9 Pa 790. 46. Seeman, ”Antidote to Alienation . . .", 22 . 933., p. 35. 47. M” p. 36. 48. 332$- 49. Rotter‘s theory would also seem to assume the existence of a paw-existing goal toward which the individual is oriented, and which he sees as legitimate for him to strive toward. 50. Frederick B. laisanen, ”Stability, Alienation and Change,” Sociological Mtgrlz, 4 (Winter, 1963), 18-32. 51. Seeman, 31. $1., p. 35. If movements gig have immediate or practical effects, it might be logical to suppose that Seeman would argue that they were not in fact alienated, but partici- pants in a grand and glorious scheme, i.e., they would by defini- tion be powerful, and therefore not alienated. Really, he seems to suggest that alienation can produce its own diagnosis and mtiutOe 52. Seeman, ”Alienation and learning in a Hospital setting," 21- cit. 53. Arthur G. Neal and Melvin Seeman, op. cit. 54. John P. Clark, ”Measuring Alienation Within a Social System,” Am :16 3061 10 6.1 eview 24 (December, 1959), 849-852. 55. M09 p. 849. 56. M. 57. 931., p. 850. 58. Dwight Dean, ”Meaning and Measurement of Alienation," W: 26 (October. 1961). 753-758. 59s Ibide, PO 7550 60. Seeman, ”On the Meaning . . .", 22. 311., p. 786. 61. Jan Hajda, ”Alienation and Integration of Student Intellec- tual,” Ana-162 SOOlOloflogl Bevin, 26 (October, 1961), 758-777. 620 M” p 63' M01 F 64. laisanen 65. Leon ht Ron-Peter»: 66. Insane: 67. M- 68' M01 69- 888mm, 910011.; 3‘ 31-5" Y1 10. 010‘“ I“ 0 81‘01. , 72s broth Access to kma 21 73. Hettl‘ 741 Clark 75. m, Kid “9 I (I? N e was: {(0 41:54 :‘5’ If g' 4.» E? ”fig/c? /'\ I 71 62. Ibid., pp. 758-759. 63s Ibide, PO 7590 64. Iaisanen, 22. 321;. 65. Leon Fastinger A Theory; of Cogitive Dissonance (Evanston: Row-Peterson, 1957s. 66e W‘isman, ‘92s as, pa 6e 57. Ibid.. 68. Ibid., p. 7. 69. Seeman, ”Alienation and Social Learning in a Reformatory," 32w t.; R. A. Cloward and L. E. Ohlin, Delinguencz Ed- Opmrtun- it; New York: The Free Press, 1960). 70. Cloward and Ohlin, Ibid., p. 111. 71. Srcle, 33. _c_i_’§_., pp. 712-713. 72. Dorothy L. Meier and Wendell Bell, "Anomia and Differential Access to the Achievement of Life Goals," American Sociological £221.01. 24 (April. 1959). 191. 73s NCtt1.r, 93o 23-3.." Do 675. 74. Clark, op, cit., p. 850. 75. This full listing of items was obtained from Dean personally, and are not presented in his article. 76. Clark, 31. 23.3.; Haida, 32. git. 770 333“! 22° 2.3.1" I’- 754- 78. Seeman, "0n the Meaning . . .," o . 93.1. 79. Item number one and two were taken from the article "Organi- sations and Powerlessness,” items three and four from the article ”Alienation and Social Learning in a Reformatary,” and items five, six and seven from the article ”Alienation and Learning in a Hospital Setting.” All items purportedly measure powerlessness. 800 3.0““, ”On th. Meaning e e e," .22. Cite, Do 784 81. Russell Middleton, ”Alienation, Race and Education,” American Sociological Review, 28 (December, 1963), 973-974. 82- .....Ibidn PP-973-974o 83. This table is a modification of that presented in Middleton, 23. cit., p. 975. 84c mm, 22. Cite, nettluyfle cite, ma. Jo Le Simona, ”SOD. Inter-Correlations Among 'Alienation' Measures,” Social Forces, 44 (March, 1966), 370-372. 85. Arthur Real and Salomon Rettig, ”Dimensions of Alienation Among Manual and Non-Manual Workers," American Sociological Review, 28 (August, 1963): 599.608- 86. Elmer L Analytic E1 imism Dome 1965), 768- 87. Desmond American Sc 88. 31'016 , 890 ‘e H. 1 Prejudice: (mey, 80310-300: Forces, 3g 91. Dean, 92. 31mg] 931 Kerri 088, 19 940 RObOI' he“ ’11 £ Unpublie] Stfitg Uh, 96e uh. we, 91s M911 Win “\30c3 72 86. Elmer L. Struening and Arthur H. Richardson, ”A Factor Analytic Exploration of the Alienation, Anemia and Authoritar- ianism Domain,” American Sociolc ical Review, 30 (October, 1965). 768-775. 87. Desmond S. Cartwright, "A Misapplicaticn of Factor Analysis," American Sociolc ical Review, 30 (April, 1965), 249-252. 88. Srcle, 33. g3. 89. A. H. Roberts and M. Rokeach, "Anomie, Authoritarianism, and Prejudice: A Replication," American Journal of Sociolc , 61 (January. 1956). 355-358. 90. Edward L. McDill, ”Anomie, Authoritarianism, Prejudice and Socic-Econcmio Status: An Attempt at Clarification," Social Forces, 39 (March, 1961), 239-245. 91. Dean, 23. cit. 92e Simmons, 22' Cite 93. Morris Rosenberg, Occumtions and Values (New York: The Free Press, 1957). 94. Robert C. Angell, ”Preferences for Moral Norms in Three Areas,” American Journal of Sociolc , 67 (May, 1962), 650-660. 95. Clark McPhail, ”Dogmatism, Religiosity and Alienation," Unpublished research report, Department of Sociology, Michigan State University, 1962. 96. Albert L. Rhodes, ”Anemia, Aspiration and Status," Social ____1'orm. 42 (May. 1964). 434-440. "'"""" 97. Melvin M. Tumin and Roy 0. Collins, ”Status Mobility and Anomie: A Study in Readiness for Desegregaticn,” British Journal 0; Sociology, 10 (1959), 253-267. 98. John D. Pactiadis and Jeanne Biggar, ”Religiosity, Education, and Ethnic Distance," American Journal of Sociology, 67 (May, 1962), 666-672. 99. Edward McDill and J. C. Ridley, "Status, Anomie, Political Alienation and Political Participation,” Amgrican Journal of Scciglogz, 68 (September, 1962), 205-213. 100. Leonard I. Pearlin, ”Alienation from Works A Study of Nursing Personnel,” gal-1cm Sociological Review, 27 (June, 1962), 314-326. 101. Nettler, 23. cit. 102. Hajda, £2. _c_i_t_. 1o3o Seeman, 21. 9.13.0 104. These studies are smarised in Clinard, 92. $3.; they are: Srcle, 2;. 2L.; Roberts and Rokeach, 23. g_i_t_.; Arthur Kcrnhauser, Harold L. Sheppard and Albert J. Mayer, When Labor Votes (New York: University Publishers, 1956), pp. 189-200; Wendell Bell, ”Anomie, Social Isolation, and the Class Structure ," Sooiometry, 20 (June, 1957). 105 Anomie: A: 1958)1 23< Op. .92., Small Cit; 545-554; 1 Charles X or Social: 3. lax Hi? (Hater, ' A Stud: 01 Rhodes, 2 1050 Nett 92' me 106. big 73 1957), 105-116: David J. Bordua, ”Juvenile Delinquency and Anomie: An Attempt at Replication," Social Problems, 6 (Winter, 1958), 230-238, Meier and Bell,.gp..gi§.; Tumin and Collins, op. 9.3.03 3. H. Mizruchi, "Social Structure and Anomia in a Small City,” American Sociolc ical Review, 25 (October, 1960), 645-654; Dean, 93. 22.3.: McDill, 22. 93.1.; Lewis M. Killian and Charles M. Grigg, "Urbanisms Race, and Ancmia," American Journal 2 of Sociology, 67 (Mb , 1962 661-665; Richard L. Simpson and H. Max MillerS "Social Status and Anomie,” Social Problems, 10 9 (Winter, 1963 256-264: E. H. Mizruchi, Success and 0 rtunit : A Study of Ancmii (New York: The Free Press, 1964); Albert L. Rhodes, 93. cit. 105. Nettler, 93. cit.; Meier and Bell, 93. cit.: and McDill, fie Cite 106e M0101: md 3011, 93s Cite, ppe 191-192e 107. McDill, 22. cit., p. 244. 108e Durkheim, 22s Cite, pe 252s 109. Seymour M. Lipset and Reinhard Bendix, Social Mobility in Industrial Society (Berkeley: University of California Press, 1960 , ppe 4"65e 110. A. B. Hollingshead, 9.7.9.12: ”Social Mobility and Mental Illness,” American Sociologicil Reziew, 19 (October,1954), 111. Joseph Greenblum and Leonard I. Pearlin, "Vertical Mobility and Prejudice: A Socio-Psychological Analysis,” Class Status, iii-d Power, eds. Reinhard Bendix and Seymour Lipset (New York The Free Press, 1953), pp. 480-491. _ 112. Peter Blau, ”Social Mobility and Inter rsonal Relations,” American Sociological Review, 21 (June,1956 , 290-295, Richard Curtis, ”Occupational Mobility and Membership in Formal Voluntary Association," American Sociological Review, 24 (August, 1959), 846-848; and Richard Curtis, "Occupational Mobility and Union Membership in Detroit: A Replication," Social Forces, 38 (October, 1959), 69-71. 113. Lipset and Bendix, 33. c_i_t_., p. 69. 114. 33.3., pp. 67-68. 115. 939., p. 253. 116. Meier and Bell, _op. gig. 117. Emile Benoit-Smullyan, ”Status, Status Types and Status Interrelations,” America Sociological Review, 9 (April, 1944), 151-161: Everett Hughes, "Dilemmas and Contradictions of Status," American Jourgil 2; Sociology, 50 (March, 1945), 353-357. 118. i. B. lental Illr. and Paul 5. Haven: Yale "The lame} and Power, “9 o Gorha Dimension - (Avast, 1 120. Rbu Consistent: 121- Waist 122' Lane} 74 118. A. B. Hollingshead and F. C. Redlich, Social Class and Mental Illness (New Yerk: John Wiley, 1958); and W. Lloyd Warner and Paul S. Lunt, The Status S stem of a Modern Communit (New Haven: Yale University Press, 1942). Also see Ruth Kornhauser, "The Warner Approach to Social Stratification,” Class, Status gd Power, eds. Bendix and Lipset, 32. gig, pp. 224-254. 119. Gerhard.E. Lenski, "Status Crystallization: A Non-Vertical Dimension of Social Status,” American Sociolc 'cal Review, 19 (Auemat. 1954). 405-513. 120. Edward E. Sampson, ”Status Congruency and Cognitive Consistency, Sociometrz, 26 (June, 1963), 146-162. 121. Waisanen, 2. cit. 122s Len.k1, 22. 0117. 123. Irwin W. Goffman, "Status Consistency and Preference for Change in Power Distribution," American Sociolc ical Review, 22 (June, 1957), 275-281. 124. James A. Geschwender, ”Status Inconsistency, Cognitive Dissonance and Social Change,” unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, IMichigan State University, 1962. - 125. B. B. Ringer and P. L. Sills, "Political Extremists in Iran,” Public Opinion Quarterly, 16 (Winter, 1952-53). 689-701. 126. Elton F. Jackson,"Status Consistency and Symptoms of Stress," American Sociological Review, 27 (August, 1962), 469-480. 127. Gerhard E. Lenski, "Social Participation and Status Crystal- lisation,” American Sociolc ical Review, 21 (August, 1956), 458- 464. 128 e GCUOhWOnder , 2£e Cit e 129. Although this finding has not yet been tested for signifi- cance, the probability that the three consistency groups in the analysis would distribute in the observed manner by chance alone 13 e16e 130. Meier and Bell, 22. cit. 131. Ibid. 132s Lenflki, 1954 md. 1956, gas 2&1. 133. Werner S. Landecker, ”Class Crystallization and Class Consciousness,” American Sociolc ical Review, 28 (April, 1963), 219-229. 134w McDill, Op. Cite, and Meier and. B011, Op. Cit. In this alize the n 9398 in th. status, at it: Bub-(13 isflfiticm future. shall the the Sup] tion of < CHAPTER II OPERATIONALIZATION OF MAJOR CONCEPTS Introduction In this chapter, the primary purpose shall be to operation- alise the major concepts presented in our theoretical hypoth- eses in the previous chapter. These concepts are: average status, status inconsistency, social mobility, alienation and its sub-dimensions of powerlessness, normleesness and social isolation, present satisfaction with life, and outlook for the future. After operationalisation of these major concepts, we shall then attend to the context and design of the study itself, the sample, analysis procedures, and conclude with a presenta- tion of our hypotheses in operational form. Average Status and Status Inconsistency Average status and status inconsistency are to some degree complementary aspects of the same concept, when operationalized. Average status usually assumes a general, underlying unidimen- sional status continuum, that may be indexed by the 'average' of a person's standing on several different component measures of status. Sometimes a simple average of positions on several different components is taken: and other times a weighted average is taken. In any case, the operationalisation of status-as-an- average also raises the possibility of variance in the status 75 896, 109:, mean for 81 or effect 6 then it is ”Palate as. general erg fication t1 I”ski: an: "”858 at In deve f“ 391881 “Stage 8‘ khaki (a “113181453 Procedure status Cc Status ( ”Cuban engages tional . 76 set, i.e., the variance of.gigg component position around the mean for all components. If each component has any validity or effect on behavior independently of this created average, then it is possible that the variance itself constitutes a separate aspect of stratification. Or, at least, this is the general argument advanced by this thesis, and by other strati- fication theorists.1 Generally, we shall endeavor to follow Lenski, and his develOpment of status inconsistency and average status. In developing these two concepts, we must first attend to the selection of the component dimensions which make up both average status and status inconsistency. .We shall follow Lenski (and others) and use occupation, education, income and ethnicity or ethnic background. we might note here that this procedure indirectly assumes that the family is the basis of status conferral. A wife, for instance, will have her average status (and status inconsistency) based upon her husband‘s occupation-unless she also has an occupation in which she engages full-time-—the family's total income, her own educa- tional level, and.her own ethnic background. This assumption has frequently been criticized, but the only available alterna- tive has been to remove from analysis all those females who do not have full-time employment.2 This, of course, would amount to almost half of the study sample, as well as half the population to which we would like to generalize. Following the selection of each dimension, a method of stand- ardizing so that . till he each 41: each 61: arrive Stat fellovj ceiure sizing the ,q “n "81 then f 13003 77 ardizing a person's rank on each dimension must be arrived at, so that a true average may be computed. This standardization will be done by developing a cumulative percentile range for each dimension, and assigning the person a percentile rank on each dimension. These percentile ranks may be then averaged to arrive at an 'average status'. Status inconsistency will be computed according to the following formula: SI -‘\/{(Xi.d. -- I”) . This pro- cedure has the effect of minimizing small deviations, and.maxi- mizing large deviations, when the 'X' is squared; then taking the square root of the resultant sum has the effect of creating an ”average variance” present in the status set (low scores would then indicate low status inconsistency and high scores, high inconsistency). Both the average status and status inconsist- ency formulas create a possible range from 0-100; due to the logical impossibility of grouped data having a percentile rank of either 0% or 100%, however, the empirical range is likely to be somewhat different. Occupition Following our notion of the family as the basic stratifica- tion unit, each person was asked for the occupation of the head of the household in which he lived and for his own occupation. Where possible, the occupation of the head of the household was utilized as the primary determinant of occupational status. In those cases where a person was retired, and had no current occu- pation, the pricr—to-retirement occupation‘was used. Again, this invs upon the of his 11 Each 8 Census, C assigned 0f prestl' Prestige Our Opera categori, the occu] distribu- host: ”when. difficul \' Pre 78 this involves the assumption that a person's status depends upon the occupation he followed (usually) for the majority of his life. Each occupation was then coded according to the 1960 U. 8. Census, Occupational Classification. Each occupation was also assigned the prestige score derived in the Duncan-Hatt analysis of prestige of occupations.4 In the Duncan-Hatt study, these prestige scores were then further transformed into deciles. Our operaticnalization assigns the mid-point of the decile categories as the percentile score (Figure 1). In summary, then, the occupational component score is the percentile standing on a distribution of occupational prestige. Prestige percentiles were used rather than percentages of occupational classifications per so, because of the general difficulty in imputing status to these general classifications, Figure 1: Derivation of Occupational Scores Prestige Score Range Percentile Score 0 - 42 05 43 - 52 15 53 25 54 - 57 35 58 - 59 45 60 - 64 55 65 - 67 65 68 - 70 75 714- 75 85 76 -'99 95 i.e., professional, technical, etc. In addition, it allows us a larger amount of variability of occupational statuses. Finally, ‘by using a prestige score which is predicted from education and income (or oorrelated.with it), we are using a score for occupa- tion whic income, a tive in 1 This vil'. discrepa IhOI up, Einsstic \ Each she had and till “03, 1 79 tion which is very likely to be consistent with education and income, at least. That is, our test will be somewhat conserva- tive in that we have built in a factor making for consistency. This will have the effect of allowing only gross and clear-cut discrepancies between occupation, and education and income, to show'up. Education Each respondent was also asked how many years of school he or she had completed. Then, in order to obtain percentile ranges and.ranks, the sample percentages in various categories were used, which produced the following range: Figure 2: Derivation of Education Scores Educational ‘ Percent Cumulative - Midpoint Level in Percentile PRS* Sample Range- None or 1-4 yrs 5.0 0 - 5.0 02 5-7 yrs 10.3 5.1 - 15.3 10 8 yrs 19.8 15e4 " 35e1 25 9-11 yrs 16.1 35.2 - 51.2 43 H.S. graduate 26.4 51.3 - 77.6 64 Technical, trade or business school 4.0 77.7 - 81.6 79 1-3 yrs college 9.6 81.7 - 91.2 86 College graduate or more 8.7 91.3 - 99.9 95 7 Percentile Rank Score. Income In order to compute income percentile rank scores, the sample statistics were again used. The question in the schedule used to elicit this information asksdihe family's total income last year. This was done in line'with our assumptions about the familial unit as the status conferral unit. The following percentile rank 3 Inc whiz r3 °°°ia1 'as him or the the“ ‘ '°uld E Van tag “hie 80 rank scores were developed: Figure 3: Derivation of Income Scores Income Percent Cumulative Midpoint in Percentile PBS* Sample Range 8 0 - 999 4.6 0 - 4.6 02 1,000 - 1,499 3.6 4.7 - 8.2 06 2,000 - 2,499 5.7 12.3 - 17.9 15 2,500 - 2,999 4.2 18.0 - 22.1 20 4,000 - 4,999 11.4 30.9 - 42.2 36 5:000 - 5,999 22.3 42.3 - 64-5 53 7:000 - 9:999 17.? 64-6 - 82.2 73 10,000 - 14,999 10.0 82.3 - 92.2 87 15,000 + 3e6 92e3 - 95e8 94 a Percentile Rank Score. Ethnicity Ethnicitwaas the most difficult dimension of all to con- struct. Generally,‘we followed Lenski's and Jackson's procedp ure for developing this dimension.5 Four ranked levels of ethnicity were posited as follows: High a 01d American, Canadian, or English b Northwest European descent 0 Southeast Eurcpean descent Low d Ne , Asian, Jewish, or Spanish—s aking finally Mexican or Puerto Rican This ranking procedure follows closely Bogardus' scale of social distances from various ethnic groups.6 If a respondent was himself, or if his parents or grandparents were, born in any of the above countries, the respondent was thusly classified. If there were parents or grandparents born in classifications which 'would have put them in different levels, the paternal lineage ‘was taken first on the assumption that ethnic identity plus ethnic surnames follow the paternal lineage more so than the maternal 1: did not km was name After 01 and cumula Percentile 81 maternal lineage. If the respondent could not remember, or did not know, where his parents or grandparents were born, it ‘was assumed that he was an "Old American". After classifications were made for the sample, percentages and cumulative percentile ranges were obtained, from which percentile rank scores were assigned as follows: Figure 4: Derivation of Ethnicity Scores Group Percent Cumulative Percentile Percentile Rank Range Lo Negro,Asian,etc. 19.0 0 - 19.0 09 S.E.Eur0pean 9e4 19e1 -' 28e4 24 N.W.European 23.0 28e5 "' 51e4 41 Old American 47e7 51e5 "' 99e1 76 Hi . Average Status——Status Inconsistency With these scores, then, each person's average status and status inconsistency were computed. If a person had any three of these scores, a computation was made: if, however, two or more were incomplete (i.e., if no information about that com- ponent was available), a computation was not made, and thus no score was obtained. Of 1,528 respondents, only 6 lacked sufficient information to compute average status or status incon- sistency. The empirical distributions ranged from 4-90 for average status, and from 1-90 for status inconsistency. ‘Examples of average status and status inconsistency which will illustrate various points along these ranges are as follows: (a) Negro M.D., making 810,000 a year Average Status 71 Status Inconsistency 73 The :11 thus deve aPFIOIima relet ions Multi-m further c and three (“9 p. 8 I“Ollow “30 int.” minty. aimPle me min 310] tlke the occu‘mtim her “the; occupatim “118.1- d1 measure ’ ‘ 82 (b) Jewish college professor making 88,000 a year Average Status 68 Status Inconsistency 70 (c) Negro insurance salesman, 7th grade education making 815,000 a year Average Status 52 Status Inconsistency 85 («1) 01d American medical technician, with technical training, making 87,200 a year Average Status 76 Status Inconsistency 4 The distributions of average status and status inconsistency thus developed were then categorized into ten categories of approximately ten percent each for the initial analysis of the relationship of each variable to alienation, etc. In the multi-variate stage of our analysis these ten categories were further collapsed into five for the average status variable, and three fer the status inconsistency variable. Table 1 (see p. 83) presents the limits of these categories. Social Mobility Fellowing our suggestions of the previous chapter, we shall use inter—generational mobility, rather than intra-generational mobility. And more specifically, we shall use a relatively simple measure of occupational mobility as our indicator of social mobility. In measuring occupational mobility, we shall take the difference between father's occupation and son's occupation: for females, we shall take the difference between her father's occupation and her husband's. In classifying occupation, we shall use a simple blue-collar versus white- collar dichotomy. we realise that this is a relatively simple measure, but there are several reasons for doing so. First, 66. 71. 77- Tote Scor 1o . 17. 24. 29 . 33. 38. 42 . 46. 52. 83 Table 1: Distributions and Collapse Limits for Categorization of Average Status and Status Inconsistency AVERAGE STATUS Score Range Ten Category Limits Five Category Limits N % N 4.- 26 147 9.7 291 19.2 35" 42 146 9.6 309 20.3 49 - 53 149 9.8 316 20.7 54 - 59 167 10.9 60 - 65 175 11.4 334 21.9 66 - 70 159 10.5 71 - 76 137 9.0 272 17.9 Totals 1,522 100.0 1,522 100.0 STATUS INCONSISTENCY Score Range Ten Category Limits Five Category';?mits N 76 N IIlIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIII-IIIIIHIIIIIIIIIIIIII 17 - 23 157 10.3 439 28.8 29 - 32 154 10.1 38 - 41 153 10.4 598 39.3 52 .. 57 164 10.8 485 31.9 Totals 1,522 100.0 1,522 100.0 there were the general demands of interview time, and to obtain intra—generational mobility requires that one plot a progression of Jobs for each respondent-a time-consuming endeavor. Second, it is not likely that a'wife‘will always be aware of her hus- band’s first Job, although she may be aware of her father's Job (or at least the Job he held during the prime years of his life). This allows us to utilize more data with more generalizability. And, I of the in tin Ope engage sional (3) 011 be Ola: line wj inter—é 8 blue. blue-cc 1 “it: 'hiteq as Stet the res °laseii the res Use Our Bar m°bile T 1312233 In 0 the D03 initial 84 And, finally, intra—generational mobility creates problems of the different prestige levels of jobs at different points in time. Operationally, we shall define white-collar workers as those engaged in the census occupational categories of: (1) profes- sional, technical and kindred: (2) managers and prOprietors; (3) clerical workers: and (4) sales workers. All others will be classified as blue-collar workers. This is generally in line with most other studies of blue-collar to white-collar inter-generational mobility.7 If the head of a household has a blue—collar occupation, and the respondent's father held a blue-collar occupation, or if the head of the household holds a white-collar occupation, and the respondent's father held a ‘white-oollar occupation, then the respondent'will be classified as stable, i.e., nonpmobile. If the father was blue-collar, and the respondent is white-collar, then the respondent will be classified as upwardly mobile: if the reverse is true, then the respondent will be classified as downwardly mobile. Use of this schema produced the following percentages in our sample: Downwardly mobile 8.65%, Stable 68%, and Upwardly mobile 23.3%. Alienation Introduction In operationalising alienation, a decision was made to use the Dean scales, with their division into sub-scales. These scales were selected because of their generality, and their initial separation into sub-scales. Because of the cross-cul- tural felt t of the Nettle desire poseib the D04 85 tural approach, as well as the use of national sample, it was felt that a general scale would be more advantageous than any of the more specific scales available at that time, e.g., Nettler's, etc.8 Also, following our conclusion regarding the desirability of attempting to separate alienation into as many possible components as possible in any study, we must note that the Dean scale is the only scale which attempts this separation. The Dean scales were not simply adopted in Mg; rather a series of Guttman scaling analyses were performed to insure the unidimensionality of each sub-scale. we must note that this was not done in the original Dean analysis, and that there was therefore no assurance of unidimensionality. This analysis would also provide us a test of the separability of the various dimensions of alienation. All in all, three separate scaling analyses were performed, with three separate pepulations: first, a student sample from Michigan State University: second, samples of the adult population of Lansing, Michigan: and last, the sub- sample of a random national sample used in the actual study. Student Sample The student sample was performed in the summer of 1962, using 100 students drawn from social psychology classes. These sapho- more-level classes were open to Juniors and seniors, as well as graduate students; in addition, summer school attracts more adults than otherwise is the case. Therefore, the sample is somewhat more heterogeneous than the usual introductory class sample of freshmen. These students were administered the full Dean scale strongly a strongly ah Isissnen t Figure ‘ harlessn 1. There a me: Dean 86 scales, with five possible response categories (Figure 5): strongly agree, slightly agree, don't know, slightly disagree, strongly disagree. The Guttman analysis used was the modified ‘Waisanen technique. 9 Figure 5: Dean's Items Used in Operationalizing Alienation Powerlessness: 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. There is little or nothing I can do towards preventing a major 'shooting'war'. we are just so many cogs in the machinery of life. The future looks very dismal. There is little chance for promotion on the job unless a man gets a break. There are so many decisions that have to be made today that sometimes I could Just blow up. It is frightening to be responsible for the development of a little child. Sometimes I have the feeling that other people are using m.e 8. We're so regimented today that there's not much room for choice even in personal matters. Normlessness: 1. I often wonder what the meaning of life really is. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. PeOples' ideas change so much that I wonder if we'll ever have anything to depend on. Everything is relative and there Just aren't any definite rules to this life. With so many religious beliefs today one doesn't really know’which to believe. I worry about the future facing today's children. The and often justifies the means. The only thing one can be sure of today is that he can be sure of nothing. Social Isolation: 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8. Sometimes I feel all alone in the world. Real friends are as easy as ever to find. PeOple are Just naturally friendly and helpful. There are few dependable ties between people anymore. I don't get invited out by friends as often as I'd really like. Most pecple today seldom feel lonely. One can always find friends if he shows himself friendly. The world in which we live is basically a friendly place. First, an attempt was made to scale all of the alienation items together, to see if, in fact, a common scale should be developed. ofv be e have than: tely rela dro] the 7211: Fig 86e: iOC. 87 This was not possible; neither a sufficiently high coefficient of reproducibility, nor an even distribution of scale types, could be achieved. Even with the removal of several items, the C.R. was not increased appreciably. Therefore, it was assumed that no general dimension of alienation existed. No combination of various sub—scales was attempted at this time. And, as will ‘be evident from our last scaling analysis, perhaps this should have been done. However, hindsight is always so much better than foresight. When each sub-scale was submitted to scaling analysis separa- tely, however, appreciable C.R.‘s were obtained, as well as a relatively even distribution of scale types. Several items were dropped so as to require an inspection of content to assure that the basic meaning of the scale had not changed. The powerlessness sub—scale achieved a C.R. of .928. Items which did not scale, and.were dropped, were items 1 and 3 (see Figure 5). Removal of item 1 regarding a 'shooting‘war' would seem to remove a more or less "political” item from the scale, i.e., an item referring to a political or international context, and thus leave the total scale a more general one. Item 3, regarding the dismal future, would seem to remove the only clearly future referent in the scale, perhaps typing the entire scale as one more oriented to the ”here and now” definition of the situation. The normleesness sub-scale achieved a C.R. of .94. with items 5, 6, and 7 being drOpped in order to meet scale criteria. This would seem to leave the total scale with a clear referent to the "u expectant; normleesnel the referz Seeman se: 1' Perhaps referring respect u re to m The 301 items 5, 7 Inspectim ‘0 the re} remaining‘ 883,19 . Adult 88.31 The it me than “tilt (ma Ea“ 14m; technicu all 0?” from Hex-t ‘ Japane r~ 1. “1% '01; aidit: 88 to the ”unpredictability of life". Item 6, which refers to an expectancy of using illegitimate means-—or the Seeman variant of normleesness——is removed, which would seem to clarify or specify the referent of the total scale as "meaninglessness" in the Seeman sense. Item 5 refers to a dismal future for children, and is perhaps more characteristic of powerlessness, because the item referring to children in the powerlessness scale remained. With respect to item 7, few "face" reasons can be seen for its fail- ure to scale. The social isolation sub-scale achieved a C.R. of .93, with items 5, 6, 7 and 8 dropped in order to meet scale criteria. Inspection of the content of the dropped items, in comparison to the remaining items, reveals little if any differences. The remaining items do, however, form a statistically unidimensional scale. Adult Sample The items which were scalable in the M.S.U. student sample 'were then tested on the following samples: (1) a sample of adult (male and female) heads of households in the Lansing and East Lansing, Michigan area; (2) a sample of persons in a technical training school in San Antonio, Texas, who were from all over South America and Central America, with the majority 10’ and (3) a.sample of adult heads of households in from‘Mexico Japan. Guttman scalingHwas carried out in an attempt to find items ‘which‘wculd meet scale criteria in all three of these samples. In addition, as many items as possible were cut out in an effort to r Evan give Cultu 89 to reduce the amount of interview time devoted to this section. Eventually, two items were selected from each scale which would give maximum representation of the entire scale. This attempt at cross-cultural scaling raised many interesting and perplexing problems in the methodology and theory of scale analysis. Generally, the most perplexing problem was whether or not to lump respondents from all samples together, or to perfbrm a scale analysis for each sample separately and inde- pendently. The decision was made to scale each sample separately, *with an attempt to use common items, item order and cutting points. If we had lumped all samples together, there would have been a serious possibility of non-random distribution of respondents in the scale analysis (i.e., respondents from one cultural sample might have clustered around a certain scale type). And, had this occurred, it would have been very diffi- cult to get any amount of variation within any one sample. That is, there was the possibility that an item would be agreed ‘with by all Japanese, and disagreed with by all Americans. This non-random distribution would not have shown up in the scaling analysis per as. Its consequences though, would have been to place every respondent in the same category for responses to that question when an analysis of that sample‘glgngboccurred, i.e., an item'would not differentiate within the Japanese pOpulation. By pursuing an independent analysis for each sample, we would check not only the C.R., but discriminability and distribution of scale ype best set c the maxim samples). the Englie modificati Wilmabi] final seal Items t Iith a mg for the J4 In Order 4 30b and m fied to N unless , t 396m to a‘ “0 item 1 t" items "1°90 It Ergo“). t to the in Item '1th 11 pe or “he: Spanish 8| 90 scale types within each sample, and between samples, until the best set of items were found (i.e., items which would produce the maximum discriminability and distribution in all three samples). Another problem which arose was the translation of the English items into Japanese and Spanish. This required modification of some items, in order to obtain maximum "meaning” comparability. Where modified items were inserted into the final scale, it will be noted. Items retained for the powerlessness sub—scale were 4 and 5, 'with a C.R. of .95 for the American sample. (The coefficient for the Japanese sample was .93 and .97 for the Spanish sample.) In order to make item 4 applicable to persons without extensive job and‘work experience, and in order to translate, it‘was modi- fied to read: "There is little chance to get ahead in this life unless a man knows the right people.” This modification did not seem to affect the content or the scalability of the remaining two item scale. An inspection of the content of these remaining two items, compared to the items out out, reveals little differ- ence. Item 4 contains a general referent, and item 7 contains a personal referent. All in all, the content would seem to refer to the inability to exert control over things that happen in life. Items retained for the normleesness scale were 1 and 2; again 'with a personal referent and a general referent. The coefficient of reproducibility was .93 for the American sample, .95 for the Spanish sample, and .88 for the Japanese sample. Again, the :referent of the total scale would seem.to be the lack of order and predictability in life. With the cancellation of item 4, referring two items previous] Items 2, vith a Simish 8 items hav to be 1,0 Japanese. seem to a "I feel a fiCtual 80. Tags, J 1119, and 1 'hen t2 91 referring to the multitude of religious beliefs, the remaining two items would seem to have a more general referent than previously. Items retained for the social isolation sub-scale were 1 and 2, with a C.R. of .97 for the American sample, .94 for the 11 Again, the Spanish sample, and .77 for the Japanese sample. items have one general and one personal referent. Item 4 had to be dropped due to translation problems, both in Spanish and Japanese. The content of the remaining two item scale would seem to apply both to a sense of loneliness and isolation-— "I feel all alone in the world"-—as well as to a feeling of actual social contact with persons. These items were included in the final version of the sched- ule, and were administered to national samples of five nations. Sub-Sample of Achieved Samplg_ When the sample of 1,528 respondents had been completed, and the interviews returned, a sub-sample of 150 was drawn (every tenth interview, with a random starting point), and a third Guttman analysis was performed. This last analysis used the same item orders and cutting points as had been arrived at in the previous analysis; thus this last analysis formed.more of a vali— dation and check on our previous analyses than an independent effort at scale construction. In this third analysis, not only coefficients of reproducibility were figured, but also the ”improvement over chance.”12 For the normleesness scale, the C.R. . .912, and the I.O.C.- .81; for and for ‘ 1.0.0. I sub-scale are an ac tion. W: 'hich ca: In ad: attempt 1 of items \ three 811 relative and norm e 0.3. o Purposes finsl Beale, Mantle and 0 r sealing Of the In re and 250.2; 39331;“: I devel Op 92 .81; for the powerlessness sub-scale, C.R. - .90 and I.0.C.- .78; and for the social isolation sub-scale, C.R. - .90 and the I.O.C. - .71. ‘We would assume from these values that the three sub-scales do have some validity and unidimensionality, and thus are an adequate specification and operationalisation of aliena- tion. ‘With two items, each scale develops three scale types, ‘which can be labeled High, medium and Low powerlessness, etc. In addition to the above scaling checks, a final heuristic attempt was made to derive a general alienation scale, composed of items from all three sub-scales. The attempt, using all three sub—scales, again failed, as C.R.‘s and I.O.C.'s were relatively low. However, a combination of the powerlessness and.normlessness sub-scales did meet scaling criteria, with a C.R. of .91, and an 1.0.0. of .66. Therefore, for heuristic purposes, if nothing else, this scale was included in our final analysis. It is termed the powerlessness—normlessness scale, and being composed of four dichotomized items has five possible values, from 0-4, with 4 representing high alienation, and 0 representing low alienation. 'We also take this latter scaling effort as some evidence of the empirical separability of the social isolation component. Life Satisfaction and Future Outlook In response to the suggestions of Meier and Bell, Nettler and McDill regarding the Srcle scale (and possibly alienation) measuring 'despair' rather than alienation, we thought to develop a more or less independent and general measure of despa: alien: develc of Opt think . 3083“) himeel: 93 despair cr dissatisfaction with one's life. 13 Further, because alienation sometimes has a future connotation, we thought to develop some measure of one's outlook for the future, in terms of Optimism or pessimism. A general technique for this sort of measure is the Cantril Self-Anchoring satisfaction scale-or at least a modification of it.14 Briefly, this technique asks the respondent to describe, in detail, the best possible life that he could imagine; and then the worst possible life imaginable. Then, the respondent is asked to consider what he has described as the best possible life as standing at the 'top' of a ladder with 11 rungs (0-10), and the worst possible life at the bottom. The respondent is then shown a picture of such a ladder and asked where he would place himself on the ladder as of right now; where he thinks he stood five years ago, and where he thinks he will stand five years from now. By having the respondent describe the polar ends of the scale, Cantril wOuld suggest that these ends are then 'anchored' in the respondents definition of his life. In a sense, Cantril is taking many diverse views of what the best and worst possible life constitutes, and is then equating them by letting them form the extreme ends of a common continuum-the ladder. Our modification of this general technique was to ask the respondent to 'imagine' the best possible life that he could think of as standing at the top of the ladder, and the worst possible life he could imagine at the bottom, and then to place himself on the ladder as of right now, etc. Our primary modi- *7 l fication, best and them to The I tee uti? to ehta respond his in: the pn lover and, 1 define ofo Thes, 94 fication, then, consisted of asking the persons to imagine the best and worst possible lives, rather than actually describing them to the interviewer in detail. The respondent's actual placement of himself on the ladder was utilized as the score for 'present life standing'. In order to obtain some indication of his outlook for the future, the respondent's present standing on the ladder was compared with his imagined future standing; if the future was higher than the present, he was termed ”Optimistic"; if the future was lower than the present, then he was defined as ”pessimistic"; and, if the future was about the same as the present, he was defined as ”neutral”. Operationally, we subtracted the respondent's score for present standing from his score for future standing, and, in order to remove any negative numbers, added a constant of 10. This then produced a possible range of 0—20. Then the following collapse limits were observed: 0 ~18 Pessimistic, with the future standing two or more steps below the present. 9 -11 Neutral, with the future within one step, above or below the present. 12 -20 Optimistic, with the future two or more steps above the present. In this fashion, a person with a present standing of ten, and a future standing of ten, was classified as "neutral", as ‘was a person with a present standing of 0, and a future standing or Go Study Context These operationalisations were carried out as a part of the 1 Five Nat: cross-cull and syete was a sur samples 0' Costs 31: the Unit. study in modifies Using necessau SChedul. tions w. uP seal The inc the so etc., ' The in that And, in necegBar and refe “r9 Sac require: Anot): Undertaj require: 95 Five Nations Project at Michigan State University—-a much larger cross-cultural study of alienation, attitudes toward change, and systemic linkage. The primary method of data collection was a survey-type, one-hour interview administered to random samples of five different nations-United States, Mexico, Costa Rica, Japan and Finland. For this problem, however, only the United States data were used.15 The inclusion of a small study in such a large undertaking'made numerous concessions and modifications necessary for the sake of the project as a whole. Using a survey approach, for example, meant that all of the necessary questions had to be fitted into a one-hour interview schedule. This required the sacrifice of many items and ques- tions which otherwise would have been beneficial. Items making up scales and other indexes were kept to an absolute minimum. The inclusion of the Japanese sample set the upper limit for the time schedule-due to the necessity of many status formalities, etc., which were required in Japanese interviewing procedures. The cross-cultural approach also presented its difficulties in that questions had to have cross—cultural applicability. And, in order to obtain this comparability, it was frequently necessary to resort to a relatively general level of meaning and reference. In the case of attitude scales, many items 'were sacrificed before finding those which satisfied scale requirements, cross-culturally. Another requirement made necessary by the very size of the undertaking was the use of computer facilities. This, in turn, required relatively structured questions, etc., for ease in coding, compute In a by the The int the Gal by thei '83 als the Fiv. the Fiv 96 coding, and working out procedures which would be feasible for computer application. In any case, the final interview schedules were worked out by the Five Nations Staff, of which the author was a part.16 The interviews in the United States were contracted out to the Gallup Organization of Princeton, New Jersey, and were done by their staff between September 2 and October 6, 1963. Coding was also done by the Gallup staff, from codes constructed by the Five Nations Staff. The analysis, however, was left up to the Five Nations Staff. Sample The sample-—as well as the interview survey—ewes done by the Gallup Organization of Princeton, New Jersey; it was composed of 1,528 persons, age 21 or older. We shall quote from their report as to the design of the sample: The design of the sample is that of a probability sample down to the block level in the case of urban areas, and to segments of townships in the case of rural areas. After stratifying the nation geographically and by size of community in order to insure conformity of the sample with the latest available estimate of the Census Bureau of the distribution of the adult population, 143 different sampling points or areas were selected on a strictly random basis, with probability of selection proportional to pOpu- lation size. An additional sample of 58 sampling points were drawn in the same manner from the states of Texas, New Mexico, Arizona, California, and Colorado. This proced- ure, in effect, doubled the number of sampling points drawn for these five states. Approximately ten interviews were conducted in each such randomly selected sampling point with the exception of the sampling points in the five Southwestern states mentioned above in each of which approximately five interviews were conducted. The interviewers had no choice whatsoever con- cerning the part of the city or county in which they con- ducted their interviews. Interviewers were given maps of the area to which they were assigned, with a starting point indies t sac to sel method folloe Sine sample 21 or is, ex gious tions) for th people a samp in the The (East, males The ed the for '6 must :- mention“ Of 5mm; exact amc “Bissau: 1* sitar~ While variables hes, fer U03 . sumf’J levela a: At the M513 ‘ 1 conriden: 97 indicated, and required to follow a specified direction. At each occupied dwelling unit, interviewers were instructed to select respondents by following a prescribed systematic method, and by a male-female assignment. This procedure was followed until the assigned number of interviews was completed. Since this sampling procedure is designed to produce a sample which approximates the adult civilian population (age 21 or older) living in private households in the U.S. (that is, excluding those in prisons and hospitals, hotels, reli- gious and educational institutions, and on military reserva- tions), the survey results can be applied to this population for the purpose of projecting percentages into number of people. The manner in which the sample is drawn also produces a sample which approximates the pepulation of private households in the United States. The returned sample was balanced within each of four regions (East, Midwest, South, and West? by educational attainment by males and females, separately. The sample is then, by definition and operation, representa- tive for the characteristics of age, sex, education and region. we must note that it is also more representative for the five mentioned Southwestern states-because of the additional number of sampling points-than for the rest of the nation, but the exact amount is not known. This could not help but increase the representativeness of the sample as a whole, and in no way could it adversely affect representativeness. While the sample is representative for the above-mentioned variables, it would be worthwhile to examine the representative- ness for other selected variables. To this end, we present the tables below. All figures listed as "census" come from the U.S. Summary of General Population Characteristics; confidence levels and.margins are taken from the Gallup Report. At the .95 percent confidence level, with percents near 90, and N - 1,500, sample error margin is 1; 2%. For whites, the confidence interval is 85.8% - 89.8%, which includes the census iSHCe r9380} from 1 the 05 For lb chané’e At 7: J 98 Table 2: Representativeness of Sample for Race Nondwhite 12.2 11.4 figure. Therefore, we would conclude that the sample is repre- sentative as far as the variable of race. Table 33 Representativeness of Sample for Size of Place Location Sample % Census % Urban 7008 6909 Rural 29.2 30.1 At the .95 confidence level, for percents near 70, with N u 750, sampling error margin 13.3. 4%» For urban, the confi- dence interval is 66.8% - 74.8%, which includes the census figure. Therefore, we would conclude that the sample is representative as far as the variable of size of place. Because of inflation, income is subject to much more rapid change than any of the other variables listed above. For this reason, we have made an extrapolation from 1959 (when the census 'was taken) to 1963 (when the sample was taken) by estimating the four—year increase in income as a fraction of the change from 1950-1960. During this ten-year period, the percent in the category, 84,999 or less, decreased 31.4%, or 3.14% per year. For the four years from 1959-1963, then, we would expect a change of approximately 12.6%. At the confidence level of .95, with the percent near 40, N - 750, sampling error is 1; 4%. For income, the confidence level for 84,999 or less is 37.9% - 45.9%, which includes the Ta Incom g 34,99 5,00 ‘ census f repreeen we we aentativ this Sam Introd In on relatio t0 exert pl“ions r°3tric1 “tiling nor Gene “mall, the MI 99 Table 43 Representativeness for Sample for Income Income Sample % Census % 1959 1953 $4,999 or 1983 41e9 52e2 "’ 12e6 . 39e6 5,000 or more 58e1 47e8 + 12e6 - 60e4 census figure. Therefore,'we would conclude that the sample is representative as far as the variable of income. we would conclude from the above that the sample is repre- sentative of the 0.8. population, and as such, findings based upon this sample will be generalizable to that population. Analysis Introduction In our theoretical chapter, we have argued that the inter- relationships among our independent variables make it necessary to exert some form of control. Further, we have argued that the previously used controls reduced the sample size, and thereby restricted generalizability. Therefore, we shall endeavor to utilize a method of control which does not restrict sample size nor generalizability. we have set upon that method of control usually referred to as 'multi-variate analysis', and have incor- porated the excellent models of Lazarsfeld and Hyman in our analysis procedures.19 Multi-variate analysis is an appropriate tool to use in situations where one is examining relationships between three inter-related variables. Literally, it is a method of examining the relationship of two variables, while holding variance due to a third variable, more or less constant. If, for example, A and 100 B, B and C, as well as A and C are related, then one may have reason to suspect that the relation of any two variables is spurious, in reality being caused by the relationship of the other two, e.g., the relationship of B to C is spurious, caused by the relationship of A to B and A to C, etc. In order to see if B is related to 0, independent of the relationship of A to B and 0, one must figure out some way of holding the effect of A constant, or removing its effect upon B and C. Partial correlations are one method of so doing, in that they mathematically remove the variance in one variable due to another, and then allow one to examine the effect of a third variable upon the remaining variance. However, partial corre- lations Operate upon the entire variable at once, i.e., operate in terms of individual scores and assume a more or less linear relationship. And, due to the fact that they operate in terms of individual scores, it is relatively difficult to obtain a table (i.e., a visual method of inspecting curvilinearity). Therefore, one cannot ascertain the differential effects of a control variable, i.e., detecting a relationship of two vari- ables which Operates at one level of a control variable, yet not at another. Multi-variate analysis, however, in that it operates in terms of grouped data—-grcups of individuals-does allow for this sort of inspection. Multi-variate analysis has the dis- advantage of losing precision (compared to partial correlation) because of this grouped data. In grouped data, one is working, in effect, with the correlation of group»means (or mean variance) of the g: is workir a group I and the Variance the true ‘e h Variabl and ate °UIVili aietenc IGVelg Bell a, 101 of the group as a whole; whereas in partial correlation, one is working with the correlation of individual scores. Moreover, a group mean is much less sensitive than an individual mean, and the group mean contains, by definition, much more random variance; consequently, it has a less reliable estimation of the true population mean. we know, however, two facts about the relationships of our variables. First, via the logical relationship of average status and status inconsistency, we know that there is likely to be a curvilinear relationship between average status and status incon- sistency, with the most extreme status levels having the lowest levels of status inconsistency. Secondly, from the Meier and Bell article regarding the relationship of social mobility and anomia, we know that social mobility may be related differently to anomia at different levels of status.20 There is also another reason for using multi-variate analysis over and above the correlation methods. Correlations must at least have interval data; our data are not interval but are ranked at most. While arguments could be advanced for their intervality, it would be more conservative to utilize a less powerful test. Also, we must note the exploratory nature of our study. If there is a true difference involved, we must assume that it will become apparent. Given the logic and inquiry of science, it is far better to reject a valid hypothesis than to accept an invalid hypothesis and thereby place it into our body of theory. If we do, in fact, reject a valid hypothesis, it is still present in nature, and given more scientific inquiry, it Iill surc veriate 1 Procedurc The f: relation: related. and on ti '8 should ship of 1 03: notic Second dent Var: Orientatj tiOn, If the conte lien..0r (Or, at J criptiOn related, frOm deg] SPGCificj our t: relatiOnc able. catQSOry as to flat 102 will surely come to light again. Therefore, we shall use multi- variate analysis. Procedures The first step in our analysis will be to test the inter- relationships of our independent variables to see if they are related. Logically, we have demonstrated that they should be, and on this basis suggested certain methods of control. Now, we should verify our contention, and check the actual relation- ship of these independent variables. If they are related, then our notions regarding the necessity of control are validated. Second, we should test the inter-relationships of our depen- dent variables, to see if "Present Life Standing" and "Future Orientation”, as indicators of despair, are related to aliena- tion. If they are related, then we have added plausibility to the contention that despair forms an integral part of aliens» tion-or that alienation forms an integral part of despair. (Or, at least we will have added another element to the des- cription of alienation.) If these dependent variables are not related, then we have indicated the independence of alienation from despair; and we then have proof of the current relative specificity of our alienation measures. Our third analytic step'will be to ascertain the first order relations of each independent variable to each dependent vari— able. In this set of tables, we will utilize the full ten- category range of status inconsistency and average status, so as to detect any curvilinearity in the relationships. we must note, howeve a test of t}: be done only presence or “lecte not] The four each iniepe; centralling reletionehi ”3W8 inoo Step, ‘8 vi able t0 the miable. j hmtheeis Finally miahle t °ther m all three I“ the be e339“: dependent “rim, . “in to Ship. Th table in“ ( "ails pt __ __ _‘._ -_.-...__ 103 note, however, that these first order tables do‘ggt‘constitute a test of the original hypotheses stated in Chapter I; this can be done only with the controls instituted. At this level, the presence or absence of a relationship neither confirms nor rejects nothing. The fourth analytic step will be to test the relationship of each independent variable to the dependent variables, while controlling for another of the independent variables-i.e., the relationship of average status to alienation, controlling for status inconsistency, then for social mobility, etc. In this step, we will examine the relationship of the independent vari- able to the dependent variable, at each level of the control variable. Analysis at this level will constitute a test of hypothesis. Finally, we will examine the relationship of each independent variable to each dependent variable, while controlling for the other‘tzg.independent variables. This step is necessary because all three variables have been shown to be logically inter-related. In the tables presented at each level of analysis, there will be essentially two steps. As the‘ggggt step, the means of the dependent variable for each value or level of the independent variable will be inspected, as well as the percentages, in order to determine the "pattern” or direction of the relation- ship. This step generally allows one to make statements about the "theoretical meaning” of the table, and thereby makes the table interpretable; if no pattern is present, then the table (while possibly statistically significant) has no theoretical messing. step—s ste present, t" statistic possibly 1; about such use at less ssmtive 104 meaning. If theoretical meaning is present, then the second step-a statistical test-dwill be attempted; if no pattern is present, the statistical test will not be attempted. The statistic used will be chi square. Although our data can possibly be interpreted as rank order data, there are questions about such an interpretation. Therefore, we have decided to use a less powerful and efficient test, in favor of a more con- servative one. There are also pragmatic factors involved in this decision, revolving around the use of the computer. It will be remembered that, due to the size of the over-all project, the computer was necessary. Although computers are reputed to be capable of anything, it turns out to be quite a laborious process to develop programs for certain rank order statistics, e.g., KolmogorovuSmirnoff, etc. Rather than go through this labori- ous-and expensive-process, it was decided to use a chi square and other statistics capable of being used at a nominal level of measurement. Related to the problem of statistics, there are certain other statistical problems contained in the use of a complex multi- ‘variate analysis procedure. Generally, these have to do With the lack of independence of observations from one multi-variate table to another. When, for example, one creates a table for the relationship of Y to Z, at various levels of control for X, this table contains a series of £913 (values of the dependent ‘variable and total) which are identical for a table listing the relationship of X to Z, controlling for Y. Only the order of «I 105 Figure 68 Lack of Independence in Two Multi-Variate Tables Using the Same Controls Table I Table II Control Indep. Dep. Control Indep. Dep. variable variable variable Variable X Y Z Y X Z r s t * r s t * a 45615; a ‘IA 45615 A b 7 8 9 24 B 13 14 15 42 10 11 12 33 A 7 8 9 24 b a 13 14 15 42 q B 16 17 18 51 B b 16 17 18 51 A 10 11 12 33 c 19 20 21 60); 423B 19 20 21 60 e Marginals. the rows is changed. From this example, it is obvious that the two different types of controls do not change one set of marginals, i.e., the right hand totals; nor do they shift the extremes, i.e., the upper- and lower-most rows. This would, we argue, create a lack of independence between the two tables, so that two sets of statis- tics, assuming independence, could not be legitimately run for both tables-or at least an interpretation based upon one signif— icant statistic would be risky. Literally, we would expect a certain number of significant statistics to occur with a higher proportion of times by chance alone than if there were true independence. In order to interpret the significance of statis- tics oomputed for the above set of tables, a distribution of statistics would be needed which would take this 'lack of inde- pendence' into account. Because such a table is not now in I existence, patterns th form. The: of control, iCent iindi the usage hsve each as halting a Becfiuse in the Sta the fOllow AS SI 535 P N 801 PM PS F0 ‘8 hypotht I. A. A 8; 0n : rel; 8C0: B. A 8 0n j Shi 0n Lit P TT ‘0 a I 106 existence, we would argue that it is much better to inspect the patterns that the various combinations of rows (and their means) form. That is, if a stable pattern forms across several levels of control, then we would be prepared to accept it as a signif- icant finding. we might note that this is also an argument for the usage of as many levels of control as possible-that is, have each variable contain as many values as possible, as well as having a relatively large sample in any study using a multi- variate type of analysis. Operational Hypotheses Because of the relatively high amount of redundancy occurring in the statement of Operationalized hypotheses, we shall utilize the following set of abbreviations: AS - Average Status SI - Status Inconsistency SM - Social Mobility P - Powerlessnsss (sub-scale of alienation) N - Nermlessness (sub-scale of alienation) 801- Social Isolation (sub-scale of alienation) PN - Powerlessness—Normlessness Combined (sub-scale of alienation) PS - Present Standing on Ladder F0 - Future Orientation We hypothesize: I. A. A significant difference, between AS groups, in the scores on P, N, 501, and PM, taking the form of an inverse, linear relationship, i.e.,'with the higher AS groups having lower scores than the lower AS groups. B. A significant difference, between AS groups, in the scores on PS and F0, taking the form of a direct, linear relation- ship, i.e., with the higher AS groups having higher scores on PS and F0 than lower AS groups. II. Little or no difference between SI groups in the scores on ”Do no in .e | big on ‘., IL IV. Mt I encl rem B.At enc re: V. AeAt er: (1: th; 81‘: dry 107 III. Significant differences between the SM groups of ”Downward", and "Upward and Stable", but little or no difference between ”Upward" and "Stable" SM groups, in the scores on P, N, SOI, PN, PS and F0. Further, we hypothesize that the "Downward" group will have higher scores on P, N, SOI and PN, with lower scores on PS and F0, than either the "Upward" or ”Stable” SM groups. IV. A.At all levels of control for SI we expect the differ- ences between AS groups hypothesized in I.A. above to remain constant. B.At all levels of control for SM, we expect the differ— ences between AS groups hypothesized in I.B. above to remain constant. V. A.At all levels of control for AS, a significant differ- ence between SI groups: (1) In the scores on P, N, SOI, and PN, taking the form of a direct linear relationship, with the higher SI groups having higher scores than the lower SI groups. (2) In the scores on PS and F0, taking the form of an inverse linear relationship, with the higher SI groups having lower scores than the lower SI groups. - B.At all levels of control for SM, a significant differ- ence between SI groups: (1) In the scores on P, N, SOI and PN, taking the form of a direct linear relationship, with the higher SI groups having higher scores than the lower SI groups. (2) In the scores on PS and F0, taking the form of an inverse linear relationship, with the higher SI groups having lower scores than the lower SI groups. VI. A.At all levels of control for AS, a significant differ- ence between all SM groups in the scores on P, N, SOI, PN, PS, and F0. Again, we expect the "Down” SM group to have the highest scores on P, N, SOI, and PN, and the lowest scores on PS and F0; and the "Stable" SM group to have the lowest scores on P, N, SOI, and PM, and the highest scores on PS and F0. B.At all levels of control for SI, a significant differ- ence between all SM groups, in the scores on P, N, SOI, PN, PS, and F0. Again, we expect the "Down" SM group to have the highest scores on P, N, SOI, and PH, and the lowest scores on PS and F0; and the "Stable" SM group to have the lowest scores on P, N, SOI, and RN, and the highest scores on PS and F0. VII. A VIII. VII. VIII. IX. 108 At all levels of third order control, with SI as the primary control, and SM as the secondary control, the differences between AS groups hypothesized in I.A., on preceding page, to remain constant. At all levels of third order control, with AS as the primary control, and SM as the secondary control, the differences between AS groups hypothesized in V.A.(1 and 2) and V.B.(1 and 2) to remain the same. At all levels of third order control, with AS as the primary control, and SI as the secondary control, the differences in SM groups hypothesized in VI.A. and B. remain constant. 1. Gerhard Dimension Menu, 1; 20 filter tions in S °rlfitslliza 3? Nhtrac 115stion, Sistency . 4' Briefl °°°upatio 310a ma] the OCCu] 3Vallab1, 101' occuj tion! 38 510a eqm prefitige no 131681;; FOOTNOTES - CHAPTER II 1. Gerhard.E. Lenski, "Status Crystallization: A Non-Vertical Dimension of Social Status,” American Sociolc ical Review, 19 (August. 1954). 405—513. 2. Walter B. Watson and Ernest A.T. Barth, "Questionable Assump— tions in the Theory of Social Stratification," Pacific Sociological Review, 7(Spring, 1964), 10-16. 3. This is a modification of Lenski's original formula, which was aimed at operationalizing status crystallization, e.g., status crystallization: ‘ A - .. " 2 10° Vgqu . xij) ° By subtracting from 100, a low score would indicate high crystal- lization, or in our obverse Operationalization, low status incon— sistency. In practice, then, they are similar formulas. 4. Briefly, this study predicted the prestige scores of all occupations listed in the census, based upon a multiple regres- sion analysis of the correlation between education and income of the occupations for which North-Hatt prestige scores were available. That is, Duncan and Hatt correlated prestige scores for occupations with the income and education of those occupa— tions as listed by the census. From this, a multiple regres- sion equation was developed which was then used to predict prestige scores for census occupations which previously had no prestige ranking. 5e Leani, _O_Ee Me, Ppe 406-407. 6. Emory Bogardus, ”Race Reactions by Sex,” Sociology and Social Research, 43 (August, 1959), 439-441. This ranking of various ethnic groups by social distance does not contradict our ranks ing in that every ethnic group classified by our method as "low", for example, ranks below every higher group in the Bogardus scale also. 7. Seymour M. Lipset and Reinhard Bendix, Social Mobility in Industrial Society (Berkley and Los Angeles: University of California Press, 1960). 8. Gwynn Nettler, "A.Measure of Alienation,” American Sociological Review, 22 (December, 1957), 670-677; Dwight Dean, "Meaning and IMeasurement of Alienation,” American Sociolc ical Review, 26 (October, 1961), 753-7580 9. Frederick B. waisanen, "A.Notation Technique for Scalogram Analysis," Sociological Quarterly, 1 (November, 1960), 245-252. 10. This sample was carried out by ClarkchPhail as part of his dissertation analysis, "Self Identification Within a Specific 109 E Context 0:1 11. In th: 50031 lac not midi: )2. knew amigo n: 18 the 831C “Whigs used: C. 110 Context of Experience and Behavior” (unpublished Ph.D. disserta- tion, Michigan State University, 1965). 11. In this latter effort, it became clearly apparent that social isolation-for the Japanese-dwas unscalable and therefore not unidimensional for that culture. 12. Improvement over chance was figured by first computing the average minimum marginal reproducibility (taking the average of the largest marginals for each item, on the assumption that this is the amount of response which could be predicted without any knowledge of the scale patterns); then, the following formula was used: C.R. __ M.M.R. ‘which gives an estimate of the percent- 1 "" MeMeRe age increase in predictability obtainable by using the scale pattern over the marginals. J.M. Jackson has suggested that an arbitrary figure of .70 be required as defining a sufficient improvement over chance. JQM. Jackson, "A Simple and More Rigorous Technique for Scale Analysis," in A Manual for Scale Analysis, Part II (Montreal: McGill University, 19495, mimeo- graphed. 13. Meier and Bell, 32. cit.; Nettler, gp.cit.; and Edward L. McDill, ”Anomie, Authoritarianism, Prejudice and Socio-Economic Status: An Attempt at Clarification," Social Forces, 39 (March, 1961), 239-245. 14. Hadley Cantril, "Hopes and Fears for Self and Country,” American Behavioral Scientist, 6 (October, 1962), supplement. 15. The study was sponsored by a variety of agencies and founda- tions: The Carnegie Foundation, the United States Public Health Service, the International Programs of Michigan State University, and the Agricultural Extension Service of Michigan State Univer- sity. 16. The directors of the Five Nations Project are Frederick laisanen, Hideya Kumata, and Charles P. Loomis; Robert L. Stewart ‘was acting director from September, 1963 to September, 1964; other staff members were Clark McPhail (1962-63). Jon Reeves (1963-64), Charles w. Tucker (1962—66), Cay Bettinghaus (1963- 64), Robert 0. Turley (1964-66), and Richard A. Brymer (1962-64). 17. Gallup Organization, Inc., He rt to the De artment of Sociolc and Anthro lo Michi an State Universit (March, 1934): PP- 3-4- 18. U. 8., Bureau of the Census, Summary of General Pepulation Characteristics: 1260. 19. Paul Lazarsfeld, "Interpretation of Statistical Relations as a Research Operation," in Paul Lazarsfeld and Morris Rosenberg (eds.), The Language of Social Research (New York: The Free Press, 1955); and Herbert an, Survey Design and Analysis (New York: The Free Press, 1955 . 20. Meier and Bell, 93. cit. In this develOped t hE’POtheseg Chapter 8h: Seatior. detailing Averase St hobility ( “13°11 the c the relat: set of ta‘ but Fl 11 Pl‘etatian Sectio depicting PWerlesS Poe-megs Film, or of the re CHAPTER III ANALYSIS Introduction In this chapter, we shall present our analysis of the materials developed in Chapter II, which shall also constitute a test of the hypotheses suggested at the end of Chapter I. Briefly, this chapter shall contain six sections. Section 1 shall contain a set of tables and interpretations detailing the inter-relations of the independent variables, e.g., Average Status (AS), Status Inconsistency (SI), and Social Mobility (SM). Also, we shall analyze the effect of each variable upon the other, while controlling for the third, i.e., examining the relationship of A8 to SM while holding SI constant, etc. This set of tables shall not constitute a test of hypotheses as such, but will give us information which will allow us to make inter- pretations of tables which do constitute tests of hypotheses. Section 2 shall contain a set of tables and interpretations depicting the inter—relationships of the dependent variables, i.e., Powerlessness (P), Normlessness (N), Social Isolation (SOI), Powerlessness-Normlessness (RN), Present Standing (PS), and Future Orientation (F0). Principally, however, it will consist of the relationships of PS and F0,to P, N, SOI, and PN. ‘We shall not analyze the inter-relationships of P, N, SOI, and PN because 111 or are no‘ Shohsn of Seotior order rele H)to our ences to d not in th! signifiCa Of °0uree and shat section , Idler ta" Ellis Wiserne P91153111; 112 this was done in the previous Guttman scale analysis in Chapter II, and would, therefore, be redundant. One element we will be concerned with here is the degree to which PS and F0 are, or are not, related to alienation, and whether they can be spoken of as constituting aspects of alienation. Section 3 shall contain a set of tables listing the first order relationships of our independent variables (AS, SI, and SM) to our dependent variables. we expect significant differ- ences to occur in the tables listing the AS relationships, but not in the tables listing the SI or SM relationships. Finding significant differences with respect to AS and alienation would, of course, validate our operationalizations of both alienation and stratification. Much of the information gleaned in this section will be used as background in the interpretation of later tables and sections. This section shall contain a set of tables and interpretations concerned with the second order relationships of our three inde- pendent variables to the dependent variables—-i.e., the relation- ship of one independent variable to a dependent variable, while controlling for another independent variable. This section shall constitute tests of hypotheses, and it is here that we expect that a relationship between SI and SM and the dependent vari- ables will become apparent. Section 5 shall contain our inspection of the third order relationships-i.e., the relationships of an independent vari- able to a dependent variable while controlling for the other two 14; indepen.. constitu. Final: tiOn 821 lower Telatj 1%“ \ 113 independent variables simultaneously. Again, this section shall constitute a test of hypotheses. Finally, we shall present a general summary of the findings, and our conclusions based upon those findings. Relationship of Average Status, Status Inconsistency and Social Mobility First Order Relationships In this section, we shall inspect the inter-relations of AS, SI and SM. The first relationship presented (Table 1) concerns the effects of AS and SI upon each other. The body of this table presents the N falling in each AS—SI cell. Of principal interest, however, are the AS means for each level of SI, and the SI means for each level of AS. Inspection of the SI means (row means) for each level of AS reveals that AS is curvilinearly related to SI. That is, persons in the highest and lowest AS categories have low SI means. The peak of this curvilinear relationship falls at the third AS level, or approximately the equivalent of a "lower middle" status. Empirically, then the point of highest or most extreme incon— sistency falls not at the exact middle of the status continu- um, as was suggested earlier, but somewhat lower. An inspection of the mean AS for each level of SI (the column means), however, reveals that there is a slightly inverse rela- tionship operating, with the higher levels of SI having relatively lower levels of average status. This may be a reflection of the relative concentration of highly inconsistent persons in the lower status ranges. That is, of those persons who are highly seamenHmzooZH mosses \\ trustee.» - custom cflecxplnoa. Hes—h one season .NOneolfinCOOCH Inseam no heroism firehouse. up ceases ..ooJc J2. .. mu moi.» mm.¢ ~N.m No.m om..v wtm mm.¢ untv motv mm.¢ mm.m ..KI .Hoo as. so. me. mm. an. es. en. mm. an? me. Hesse a: as: 8.8: we we. .2: E to. we to. we we. as as 114 se.. mm. Ae.r v e e m s. N e m. e .m es m..v an. Ao.m V m. a m a. mm m as me e mm mm.¢ mm. Am.opv as m. m m as ou om a. no mr.e m». “n.aav on m am m. or m. om r. as .e.m he. ho...v mm as cm a. mm mm m. mm o ev.m mew Am.m v me am m. mm m. .m om m u .~.m mes A~.o.v n. mm mm rm .m m. up a m er.n we. Ar.m v e. «N mu m e. mp PP r a mm.m era Am.m v a, no N. m. e m. m r. m m..m so. A~.m v a . m a m e um mm em on OFNmfi'mmb-(D‘m was asses g e r a. e m e m hogan" mzoozH Edam omssm endoplseeéaa on» modem .hosevsflmsoosH seesaw he. unseen ewshebd 3 capes 115 inconsistent, there are more in the lower end of the middle range than in the upper end of the middle range. The chi square for the entire table is 734, which is significant at beyond the .001 level. When the full ten-value range of both AS and SI is collapsed into the five and three category ranges to be used in the control tables, the curvilinear and inverse relationships become much more pronounced and clear (Table 1a). In any case, these tables demonstrate the need for control when examining the relationship of either AS or SI to any third variable. Table 2 demonstrates the relationship between Average Status and Social Mobility. Again of importance are the row and column means, listing, respectively, the mean SM for each level of AS, and the mean AS for each level of SM. we must comment on the concept of ”mean Social Mobility". This is, of course, an incorrect usage of the statistical device of the mean because presumably the various categories of Social Mobility cannot legitimately be ordered, i.e., they are nominal. For this reason, we have also included the percentages across, i.e., the percent of persons falling into the various SM categories at each level of AS. An inspection of these percentages across, as well as the "mean SM", for each AS level, reveals that there is a roughly linear relationship, with higher AS persons having higher proportions in the "Upwardly Mobile” category than those persons of lower AS. This is to be expected. The reverse, how. ever, is not true. Lower status persons reveal little downward mobility, with the largest concentration (94.6%) existing in the 116 Table 1a: Average Status by Status Inconsistency, for Collapsed Veluss of Average Status and Status Inconsistency Average STATUS INCONSISTENCY High 2 316 5.43 3 334 4.56 (High) 4 272 2.93 T°tel 439 598 485 i * 4.85 4.67 3.92 * .Means derived from ten-value range. Table 2: Average Status by Social Mobility SOCIAL MOBILITY - Average Ur Status N (95) N (9‘) N (fi) N (73) (Low) 0 3( 2.3) 122(94.6) 4( 3.1) 129(1oo) 2.01 Down Stable Total Row x 1 10( 7.6) 114(87.0)a 7( 5.4) 131(100) 1.98 2 11( 8.2) 109(81.3) 14(10.5) 134(100) 2.02 3 20(13.5) 112(15.7) 16(1o.8) 148(100) 1.97 4 16(11.6) 98(71.0) 24(17.4) 138(100) 2.06 5 17(11.2) 90(59.2) 45(29.6) 152(100) 2.18 6 21(12.9) 88(54.o) 54(33.1) 163(100) 2.20 7 13( 8.4) 90(58.1) 52(33.5) 155(100) 2.25 8 9( 6.9) 64(49.2) 57(43.9) 130(100) 2.37 (High) 9 2( 1.5) 73(55.7) 56(42.e) 131(100) 2.41 Total 122( 8.7) 96o(68.0) 329(23.3) 1411(100) Col. f 4.49 4.00 6.24 4.56 :2 - 191.1, or - 18, p(.001. 117 _ "Stable" category. What downward mobility there is appears to fall into the middle and lower middle levels of AS. This is, of course, paralleled by examining the mean AS for each social mobility category, which reveals that (as expected) the ”Upwardly Mobiles" have the highest mean AS; what is somewhat surprising, though, is the finding that those persons who are downwardly mobile have higher AS than those who are stable. we must remember that we are using an inter-generational measure of mobility. The chi square for the entire table is 191, which is significant at beyond the .001 level. This findingHwill affect our hypotheses, in that we should now expect that those persons in the ”Stable" category of SM will have higher alienation than either the upwardly or down- wardly mobile, strictly because of the "contamination" by the AS dimension. This points up the need for control in examining the effect of SM on alienation. Table 3 lists the relationship of Status Inconsistency and Social Mobility, and uses the full ten-value range of Status Inconsistency. Further, Table 3 indicates a small but statis- tically significant direct relationship between SI and SM, with a chi square of 34.5, which is significant .025)1p > .01. By examining the mean SI for each category of SM, we can see that the "Downwardly Mobile" have the lowest mean SI, and the "Upwardly Mobile" have the highest mean SI. An inspection of the mean SM, as W611 as the percentages across for each level of SI, reveals that there is a very roughly direct linear 118 Table 3: Status Inconsistency by Social Mobility g SOCIAL MOBILITY Down ‘Stable Up 1 2 Total Row <1) i‘ Status Inconsis. (Low) 0 8 101 29 138( 9.8) 2.15 1 12 100 28 140( 9.9) 2.11 2 14 84 22 120( 8.5) 2.07 3 17 95 26 138( 9.8) 2.07 4 13 100 22 135( 9.6) 2.07 5 14 89 43 146(10.3) 2.20 6 9 84 34 127( 9.0)' 2.20 7 14 119 35 168(11.9) 2.13 8 14 106 37 157(11.1) 2.15 (High) 9 7 82 53 142(10.1) 2.32 Total 122 960 329 1411(100) Col. '1? 4.43 4.50 5.09 4.63 x2 - 34.5, a: - 18, .025) p) .01. relationship, with the higher levels of SI having generally higher SM means, and larger percentages of persons in the "Stable" and "Upward” SM categories. This relationship does, however, produce some degree of confusion, because from Table 2 we know that the "Upward” SM category has a high AS mean-and from Table 1 we know that "High” AS persons have relatively low SI. That is, from Tables 1 and 2 we would assume that upwardly mobile persons have low SI; and in fact they have high SI. This again points up the need for control. _§gcond Order Control Relationships In this section we shall examine the inter-relationships of <3ur three independent variables, two at a time, while controlling 1 l 119 for the effect of the third. In these tables, we shall present only N and the row and column means. And, following our argur ment regarding the use of statistics in multi-variate tables, we shall not compute any statistics; instead, we shall rely upon a simple inspection of patterns of row and column means. Table 4 details the relationships of Average Status and Social Mobility, at all three levels of Status Inconsistency. Generally, we can say that the control for SI does not disrupt the linearity of mean SM for each level of AS, which was found in Table 1. That is, at each level of SI, there is a general increase in mean SM with each increase in AS level. This line- arity would appear to be somewhat clarified, however, as one moves from low to high SI control levels, i.e., the linearity is ever increasing at the high SI level, whereas there is some slight curvilinearity for the “Iow”AS levels, at the ”Low" and "Medium” levels of the SI control. With respect to the mean AS for each category of SM, however, all we can say is that the "Upward” category, at all levels of the SI control, maintains the highest mean AS. This also was present in Table 2. The mean AS of the "Stable” and "Down” SM categories, however, is changed from that in the original table, but only for the "Low” SI control level, i.e., at the "Low" SI level, "Stable"- 1.86, "Down” I 3.68, whereas at the "Medium” SI level the means are respectively, 1.86 and 1.90; and at the ”High” SI control level, 1.51 and 1.48. These compare ‘with 4.49 for the ”Down" SM, and 4.00 for the ”Stable" in the 120 original table (Table 2). A problem, then, is about elements in the "Icw"'level of SI which create approximately equal amounts of mean AS among "Up” and ”Down” SM persons. If we remember that the ”Low" SI group- ing contains both very high and low AS persons, but with a pre- ponderance of high AS persons; and that in terms of mobility, most lower AS persons are stable; then, it may be that for those of low inconsistency, mobility separates out those persons who are high and low status. That is, we know that the "Low" SI group is composed of both very high, and very low AS persons, with a relatively high AS mean. Then, these who are low status are also stable, so that only those downwardly mobile persons who are somewhat middle class are left, and they combine to form the relatively high mean AS present in the downwardly mobile group at the low SI level. In effect, we are observ- ing another form of the SIéAS relationship, as it Operates via SM. As it also turns out, the entire group of "Down" SM to "Low" SI has an N of 34, of a sample of 1,522 (or approximately 2.4%). And, of these 34, 16 fell in the next to highest AS level. Table 5 lists the relationship of Average Status to Status Inconsistency, for the three levels or categories of Social Mobility. An inspection of the SI means for each level of AS, reveals that the controls for SM do not disrupt, nor appreciably alter, the curvilinearity of SI or AS. Nor is the linear regres- sion of AS scores for each increasing level of SI altered, except :for those persons in the ”Stable" level of control. If anything, “this relationship is made much more clear in the ”Up" and "Down" mm.m mw.m mm.m N vm.w mw.w mm.P N mv.w om.w mm.m N as. we oe am «Aemv mm. me me we eflflmv am. 0 m m vfiamv em._ me as a? m mo.P em we on m mm. M Ma ea m mn.w oe mm e m ma.. mm so. mw m em.w we Ne w m em.. om o. o a mm.. are em em F mm.. me e N . mm.a a m 1. P voqv Nm. mm mm woe ofioqv mo.. m e e oflogv x swam cos :3 m4 M SE was :3 m4 x . swam son :3 3 I. AhcnopmfimsoosH unpspmv AhosopmwnsoonH mapspmv I. AhosopmwnsoosH ndpspmv e835 .Sfim damages MBHAHQOE AdHoom Mpfiafipos Hsfioom mo Hosea nosm vs honopmHmQOOSH mopspm hp mopspm cwcnobd .m canoe 121 mm.~ R; we; M mm.m mm; om; M 84 mm; 83 M ev.m m. me o efismv me.m oe me m «Afimv mm.~ an we m eAHmV ~m.m we em a m m mm.m we we mp m .o.m a. on we m mm.m oe an Ne w mo.m mm we. a. m oo.m e mm e m mo.m om are me P no.9 or em m. a mm.. 0 mm N . mo.m a mm m voqv mm.. m me e vogV em.. a mo. e OAQQV "E E g x-. as oapmpm neon me x as capapm egos me m up mapapm neon m4 Aspaaflpos Hasoomv Aspnafipos Hanoomv MonopmflmsoosH swam MonopmwmsoosH ensue: MozmemHmZOOZH mbadem. mspaanpos Hufioomv encamfimcoosH BOA hocmpmfimsoosH mopsvm ho Hc>oq doom as hpaawpos HsHoom hp nspspm cmsscbd .¢ canoe 122 levels of control, for in the original table (Table 13), there was only a small difference in mean AS between the "Low" and "Medium" levels of SI—-4.85 and 4.67, respectively. The lack of a difference in mean AS between the "Low" and "Medium" levels of SI at the "Stable" category of control may be due to the correlation of SM and AS, particularly the fact that the "Stable" group in Table 2 has the lowest mean AS of all three SM groups. That is, the "Stable" group contains disprOportionate numbers of lower class persons, who are also, by definition very low in SI (Table 1). It would appear, then, that SM might obscure the relationship of AS and SI, in the original tables. This interpretation would appear to be the case, when Table 6 (which describes the relationship of Social Mobility to Status Inconsistency, at all levels of Average Status) is inspected. That is, "Up" SM, at each level of AS, apparently creates high SI. The converse relationship is also clarified by the control for AS. In the original table (Table 3), the regression of mean SM for each level of SI was somewhat linear; in the control table, these SM means are, in all cases, clearly linear. What is not particularly clarified, however, is the difference between the "Down” and "Stable" categories of SM; at the "Low” AS level, the "Down" category has a higher mean SI than the "Stable" (1.07 vs. .82), whereas at the third and fourth status levels, the ”Stable category has a higher mean SI than the "Down" category; and the middle AS levels of "One" and "Two" retain the lack of differentiation present in the original table. It is clear, 123 3. mm. 5. M 3... No; mm. M we.m we me o swam em.~ we em m swam 23 av 3 m ems m~.~ S E. 2 so: mm.m an me m sea .o.m a. on m. sea M mp «Seem anon Hm M .5 £933 £69 Hm Aswamv e m R: w... 3.. M 3; mm; mm; M mm... 8. mo; M mm.m oe am we swam mo.w om ... m. swam no.m a mm m swam mo.m mm woe ea eos mm.. or em m. so: mm.. m me e ems oo.m v mm e :94 mm.. 0 mm m sea ~m.. F mow e seq M an 03qu 8.8 Hm M as .33.... :38 Hm M an .Sfim Eon Hm N w mDedam m0< 0 A55 mspspm omsno>< mo Hoeoq mean as thHHpos Hsfioom hp hosovmfiusoosH museum .m canes , __ Q I . i s O 9 . n. . q 1 O f . . --f .. 124 though, that the control for AS does alter the relationship of SM and SI. The previous statement might serve as a model for our summary of the relationships of our independent variables. That is, at the first order level, it is evident that there are a series of contaminated inter-relationships between AS, SI and SM. For example, we note from Table 1 that high AS persons are likely to have low SI; from Table 1, high AS persons are also likely to be ”Up” SM; this would then lead us to the tenative conclu- sion that "Up" SM persons are also likely to be low SI, because of the relationship between SM and AS. But this is not the case, as we can see from Table 3, where "Up" SM persons have relatively high SI—-the exact opposite of our prediction. At the control levels, we also saw that many of the original patterns held up; but also, some are appreciably changed. While our main point is not to clarify the relationship of these three variables, it is clear that they are highly inter-related. And, this, of course, validates our original statement regarding the necessity of control in examining the relationship of these three variables to alienation. Further, it points up the need to control at both the second and third order levels, because many of the original relationships of these independent vari- ables continued to hold at the second order. Relationship of Dependent variables In this section,‘we shall present the inter-relationships of our dependent variables. we shall present, however, only those 125 relationships between Present Standing and Future Orientation, and the set of four alienation measures, i.e., Powerlessness, Social Isolation, Normlessness, and the combined form, Powerless- ness-Normlessness. (Hereafter, we shall refer to these variables by their initials.) It will be remembered that these former variables were constructed in an effort to delineate a relatively "pure" measure of the "despair" or ”dismal future" outlook imputed to alienation by some alienation theorists. At this point, then, we would like to carry out an empirical examina- tion of the relationship between these two different "forms" or "aspects" of alienation. Tables 7, 8, 9 and 10 document the relationship of Present Standing to the alienation scales: P, SOI, H and PN. It will be recalled that P8 was measured by asking the respondent to place himself on a ladder describing his present standing between the best and worst possible lives that he could imagine. The values of PS in Tables 7-16 represent these standings. It will be noted that the vast majority of persons ranked themselves as standing at step five or above, with only 10 percent ranking themselves below five. This would seem to indicate that Americans consider themselves as having a relatively good life, or at least a "better than average" life. All tables exhibit significant chi squares, with probabilities of .01 or less. An inspection of the column means-—or the means IS for each level of alienation-reveals that there is inverse and.fairly linear regression of PS on alienation; with each :increase in alienation level, there is a consistent decrease in 126 Table 71 Present Standing by Powerlessness 15 100) 1 12 100 1.42 2 12 100 1.42 3 37 100 1.22 4 83 100 1.12 5 293 100 .99 6 212 100 .94 7 226 100 .76 8 106 36.8 114 39.6 68 23.6 288 100 .87 9 40 37.4 41 38.3 26 24.3 107 100 .87 (High)10 83 37.7 85 (38.6 52 23.7 220 100 .86 metal 505 (33.6) 613 (40.7) 387 (25.7) 1505(100) .92 0010 X 7019 6074 6.46 6.85 x2 - 46.535, 4: - 20, significant < .001. Table 81 Present Standing by Social Isolation Present Standing (Low) 0 N (%9 SOCIAL ISOLATION 1 N (High) 2 (75) (%) (16w) 0 1 20.0 8 53.3 4 26.7 15 100 1.07 1 1 8.4 7 58.3 4 33.3 12 100 1.25 2 1 8.3 5 41.7 6 50.0 12 100 1.42 3 9 24.3 18 48.7 10 27.0 37 100 1.03 4 18 21.7 36 43.4 29 34.9 83 100 1.13 5 62 21.2 167 57.0 64 21.8 293 100 1.01 6 37 17.4 119 55.9 57 26.7 213 100 1.09 7 52 22.8 121 53.1 55 24.1 228 100 1.01 8 78 27.0 161 55.7 50 17.3 289 100 .90 9 25 23.4 64 59.8 18 16.8 107 100 .93 (High)10 75 33.5 114 50.9 35 15.6 224 100 .82 Total 361 (23.9) 820 (54.2) 332 (21.9) 1513(100) .98 001. X 7.19 6085 6039 0 x2 - 44.4, df - 20, significant‘ .01. ”‘7’ 127 Table 9: Present Standing by Normlessness mamassnnss Present (Low) 0 1 (High) 2 Total Row Standing N (1%) N (76) N (9%) N (96) J! (Low) 0 3 20.0 4 (26.7 8 53.3 15 100 1.33 1. 3 25.0 1 8.3 8 66.7 12 100 1.42 2 2 16.7 2 16.7 8 66.6 12 100 1.50 3 4 11.1 13 36.1 19 52.8 36 100 1.42 4 13 15.9 32 39.0 37 45.1 82 100 1.29 5 54 18.6 117 40.2 120 41.2 291 100 1.23 6 44 20.7 84 39.4 85 39.9 213 100 1.19 7 68 30.0 97 42.7 62 27.3 227 100 .97 8 78 27.2 114 39.7 95 33.1 287 100 1.06 9 25 23.4 51 47.6 31 29.0 107 100 1.06 (High)10 62 27.9 72 32.4 88 39.6 222 100 1.12 Tota_l_ 356 (27.9) 587 (39.0) 561 (37.3) 1504(100) 1.14 001. X 7e16 6e87 6e58 6e83 x2 . 48.02, a: - 20, significant .001. PS means. All of these PS means are still,gbgzg.the middle of the ladder, however (i.e., five). An inspection of the row means-the mean alienation for each level of PS—-does not indicate a corresponding linearity, or at least the linearity is relatively rough. In the case of P, N and PH (Tables 7, 9 and 10, respectively), there is a general decrease to about PS value "7”, and then a slight increase in row means for the values ”8", "9” and "10”. This indicates that those people who rate themselves highest on the PS ladder are also slightly more alienated than those in the middle-but still much less alienated than those who rate themselves below the rung "5" on the ladder. Of all the alienation sub—scales, SOI comes closest to indicating a very rough form of curvilinearity (Table 8). The row means in this case start fairly high, but 128 .n Aaoo. .oe . me .eea.o.. . Ne Nm.e Na.m me.n Fm.e ...» mm.e m .Hoo aN.N pan. MFN mam ome emN ma. Hence FP.N AoowV maN ar.m4V mm n.aNV aw Ae.NmV we Ao..NV we Ao.o.V NN oFAnNNNV mo.N AooaV so, AN.w.V N. Ao.NNV om Ao.wNV om Ae.MNV mN Am.m V or m NF.N AooaV eNN Am.o.V om Am.eNV a» Am.mmV Po. AN.N.V Nm A..m V mN N Fm.. nooFV mNN Aw.» V a. AN..NV me Ae.mmV ow Ae.eNV mm Ae.oaV eN a MN.N AooeV NFN no.m4V em Am.eNV an Am.mNV Ne AN.NNV we Ae.m V N. e em.N AooeV .mN Ae.m?V mm AN.omV mm An.me Na. A..m.V mm Am.e V e. m em.N AooPV Nm Ao.NNV we Ae.mmV mN Am.MNV PN AN.N.V or Am.e V e e NN.N AooeV mm Am.NmV e. AN.NNV N “n.a.V a AN.eFV e AN.N V a m No.m AooaV Na “n.amV a Ae.mFV N Am.w V P Am.m V a Am.m V a N Ne.N nooaV NP Am.mmV e Am.mmV e Am.m V a Ae.eeV N Am.w V F F mm.N AooPV ma A».eeV N Am.m.V N he.mNV e Am.m?V N Ao.o V o c Asqu M $3 2 ARV z 83 z 33 z ARV 2 $3 2 $223.... 36m Hence e AnwamV m N F o AsoaV anemone mmmzmmfismoaummngfimmsom mmmammoHBHOZImmosmmoHnoscm hp msfidsspm «somonm .op canoe 129 increase until the third rung of the ladder (value 2), and then -decrease fairly constantly until PS value 10. This curve does not exhibit the slight increase for the last three values of PS as the other tables did, which leads a slight curvilinearity to the regression of SOI means for each level of PS. This may be evidence for the separation of SOI from other forms of aliena- tion. In any case, it would seem to be clear that alienation is a relatively close correlate of one's perception of where he stands in terms of achieving a best or worst possible life. And, if it is, we would expect that PS will be related to our independent variables in much the same way as alienation. Tables 11, 12, 13 and 14 detail the relationship of Future Orientation to the various sub-scales of alienation. Future Orientation (F0), as stated in the last chapter, is a measure of the discrepancy between PS and one's estimation of where he will stand on the ladder five years in the future-again between the best and worst possible lives imaginable. Those persons whose future is within one or two steps of their present were desig— nated as "Status Quo" oriented; those whose future was higher were designated "Optimistic", and those with the future lower than their present were designated "Pessimistic". In order to obtain mean F0 scores, these three categories were arbitrarily assigned the follow'weightst "Pessimistic" I 0, ”Status Quo" - 1, "Optimistic" - 2. Chi squares for all tables were significant at the .029 level or less. What is surprising, however, is that the row means (mean alienation for each F0 category) are not linear, i.e., 130 Table 111 Future Orientation by Powerlessness POWERLESSNESS Future Pessimistic (0) 33(31.1) 43(4o.6) 30(28.3) 106(100) .97 Status Que (1) 231(38.8) 228(38.2) 137(23.0) 596(100) .84 Optimistic (2) 229(3o.o) 326(42.7) 209(27.3) 764(100) .97 mots; 49303.6) 597(4o.7) 376(25.7) 1466(100) .92 001. X 1.40 1e47 1e48 1e45 x2 - 12.304, 8: - 4, .025 )1) > .01. Table 121 Future Orientation by Social Isolation SOCIAL ISOLATION (Low) 0 1 %) N (High) 2 N (%0 Future Orientation Pessimistic (0) 21(19.8) 62(58.5) 23(21.7) 106(100) 1.02 Status Quo (1) 173(28.7) 326(54.2) 103(17.1) 602(100) .88 Optimistic (2) 159(20.8) 406(53.0) 201(26.2) 766(100) 1.05 Total 353(23.9) 794(53.9) 327(22.2) 1474(100) .98 Gale X 1e39 1e43 1e54 1e45 x2 .. 23.0, df - 4, p>.001. Table 131 Future Orientation by Normlessness NORMLESSNESS Future (Low) 0 1 (High) 2 Total How Orientation H W N t N t N W x Pessimistic (o) 17(16.0) 39(36.8) 50(47.2) 106(100) 1.31 Status Quo (1) 166(27.9) 222(37.2) 208(34.9) 596(100) 1.07 Optimistic (2) 169(22.1) 312(4o.8) 283(37.1) 764(100) 1.15 Tots; 352(24.0) 573(39.1) 541(36.9) 1466(100) 1.13 001. x 1.43 1.48 1.43 1.45 12 - 12e54’ df . 4’ .025>p).01. 131 .80. A 2 moo; .o .. Ne $.91 NM me.e Ne.. me.. me.. Fe.. mn.. M..Hco oN.N AooPV awe. Am.e.V moN A..eNV .Nm Am.NmV one AN.N.V meN AN.» V e.. Hence mN.N AooFV Nee AN.mFV we. AF.mNV mm. Ao.an NeN A..N.V om? Ae.m V on “NV cascaaasao mo.N AooaV eon Ao.m.V Ne Am.eNV sea Ae.0mV am. Am.aNV NNF Ao.o.V mm A.V cad mavsvm em.N AooaV no, A..m.V we Ao.mmV mm RN.omV Nm Ao.N.V o. Ae.e V m AoV cascasasccm M g s as s as a as e 93 e as a 83.838 scm ”sacs e AsmNmV m N a 0 Asch cusssa mmmammmusmozummszmnnnmnaom mucouseHsnozlmmesneeHsekom an scavevseaso enuesh as? eafies 132 e "Optimistic" are not less alienated than the "Pessimistic", one would implicitly assume. Rather, the "Status Quo" tegory in each and every case is the least alienated, with e "Optimistic" and "Pessimistic" F0 categories having approxi- tely equally alienation means. Inspection Of the column means can F0 for each level Of alienation) adds little to the neral interpretability Of the tables. To some degree, the Durkheim-Merton rationale may be Operat- g here. That is, those persons who perceive a change in the ture-dwhich may cast doubt on the efficacy of the normative stem—-develop alienation, whereas those who perceive an cunt of stability in the future do not develop alienation. this sense, F0 is similar to SM, in that the ”Stable" or tatus Quo" group may be the least alienated, at least hypothet- ally. This finding may also, however, be spurious and a atistical artifact of our measurement device. In order to be ther "Pessimistic" or ”Optimistic”, a person must have a ore between two and eight, i.e., for "Optimism", a person uld score no higher than sight on the PS ladder, in order for m to have statistical ”room" to increase. And, this is a wer score when compared to persons in the nine and ten ladder nks, which in turn would give him a comparatively higher ienation score. Approximately the same dynamic can work for e "Pessimistic" persons. Further, we must note that the jority of persons in the F0 tables scored in the "Optimistic" tegory. Therefore, in order to have a resolute test of this, s should control for PS in examining the relationship Of F0 to 133 alienation.1 In any case, it is clear that PS may be an integral part of alienation, but that F0 is not; or at least F0 is partially inde- pendent, and may be related to our independent variables in different ways than alienation. It does form a definite measure of future orientation. For these reasons, then, we would suggest keeping F0 in our analysis. First Order Relationships Relationship of Average Status to Dependent variables In this section, we shall examine the relationships of Average Status tO the dependent variables, which are listed in Tables 15- 20. In these tables, AS is presented in its full ten-value range, in order to give us as full an insight as possible. In the later control tables, AS will be reduced to a five-value range. An inspection of Tables 15, 17 and 18 reveals that Average Status is significantly related to P, N, and their combination, PM, with all chi squares having a probability of .001 or less. This relationship is generally linear, and inverse, with each higher AS level having a lower mean alienation than lower AS levels. The only exception to this general finding occurs in the ninth AS level (8), where the mean alienation is somewhat above the inferred regression line. Social Isolation, on the other hand, is not significantly related to AS, although the relationship is in the predicted direction, i.e., inverse. Again, this is an independent vali- 134 Table 151 Average Status by waerlessness 1 Powaansssnass Average (low) 0 1 (High) 2 Total Haw Status N (V N (%9 N (%) N (%J x 0 6 41 28.5 82 56.9 144 100 1.42 1 2 54 37.5 48 33.3 144 100 1.04 2 5 72 49.7 36 24.8 145 100 .99 3 46 28 6 69 42.8 46 28.6 161 100 1.00 4 41 27.7 72 48.6 35 23.7 148 100 .96 6 65 37.4 70 40.2 39 22.4 174 100 .85 7 78 49.4 53 33.5 27 17.1 158 100 .68 8 47 34.6 61 44.8; 28 20.6 136 100 .86 (High) 9 70 51.8 53 39.3 12 8.9 135 100 .57 Total 506(33.5) 617(40.8) 389(25.7) 1512(100) .92 2 X - 143e22, d: ' 18’ P4 .mN canoe 137 Table 19 documents a significant relationship between AS and PS (the chi square has a probability of .001 or less) with the higher AS levels having generally higher PS means. The rela- tionship is not, however, perfectly linear. In fact, the lowest PS mean occurs not at the lowest AS level, but at the third level (value 2). It might also be noted that this is the level at which Status Inconsistency reaches a peak (Table 1). Perhaps this is an indication that SI is affecting the PS means. In any case, there is general validation of the notion that one's perception of his present situation is——like alienation- affected by his Average Status. Table 20 documents a weak but significant linear relationship between AS and F0, with the lower AS levels tending to have slightly less persons in the "Optimistic" category than the higher AS levels. That is, persons at the higher AS levels tend to have a more Optimistic outlook than do others. Given this relationship, we must now ask what the relationship of F0 is to alienation (remembering that F0 and alienation are not related). It may be that all (F0, PS, etc.) are separate facets of an AS alienation, which is not independent Of AS. Relationship of Status Inconsistency to Dependent Variables In this section, we shall examine the relationship of Status Inconsistency to the dependent variables of alienation, PS and F0. These are contained in Tables 21-26. We reiterate that this section does not constitute a test of our hypotheses per se, but forms a background against which to cast other tables 138 AecssassooV ..oo. on .om .. Ne .NN..NN. .. Nu “n.a.V NNN AN.N V NN AN.N V NN AN. V N. AN. V N. Ao..V N. Nance AN.N V .. AN.N V N Am.. V N AN.. V N .0. V o .0. V o m Anm.NV Ao.ONV NN AN.N V m Ac. V 0 AN. V . AN. V . .6. V o N .m.N.V 4N AN.N V e .0. V 0 Ac. V o .0. V o .0. V o N A..N.V on Ae.m V N AN.. V m “N. V . .0. V o .N. V . N AN.NNV me AN.N V N “N.. V N .0. V o .0. V 0 AN. V . m AN.NNV mm Ae.m V N A..e V N .0. V 0 AN. V . .N. V . e AN.cNV on AN.N V .. .m.N V 4 AN. V . .N. V . .m.NV e m AN.NNV mm .m.N V N. .N.N V N AN. V . .N. V . AN.NV e N AN.N.V NN Ao.N V o. Am.c V N A..N V m .N.N V c A..NV m . A..N.V NN AN...V N. AN.N V N A..N V m .N.N V e .N. V . o AsQHV E z E z E z E a E a E z 8...... m o m N N o oBQHV omoaebd NZHQZNNN szmmmn modemspm psomenm Mp asecem emsso>< .m. canoe 139 NN.N Aoo.Vo.m. AN.N.V NNN Ao.N V No. NN.N.V NNN .AN.N.V NNN A..e.V N.N Hence NN.N Aoo.me. .....V N. Am.m V N AN.NNV mm AN.NNV Nm AN.N.V N. N AsmamV oo.N Aoo.VNm. AN.N V N. AN.N V .. AN.NNV .m AN.NNV cm Am.m.V N. N NN.N Aoo.VNN. AN.N.V .N AN.N V N. AN..NV em AN.NNV Nm Am.m.V «N N ...N .oo.VNN. AN.N.V mN AN.N V N. AN.N.V mm Ao.N.V NN AN.N.V NN N NN.N Aoo.VNN. AN.o.V N. Ao.N V o. NN.N.V mm NN.N.V NN AN.N.V NN N NN.N Aoo.VNe. Ao.N.V NN Am.o.V N. AN.N.V NN AN...V N. Am.o.V N. e NN.N Aoo.VNN. AN.N.V mN Am.e V N Ao.m.V .N .N...V m. AN.e.V NN m NN.N Aoo.VeN. AN.N.V 0N AN.N V N AN.N.V NN AN.N.V N. AN.N.V oN N NN.N .oo.VNN. AN.N.V NN Am.N V N. AN.N.V NN Am.N V N. Ae.m.V N. . NN.N Aoo.VNc. AN.0NV on ...N V N AN.N.V NN AN.e VfN An.N.V N. o Aaqu M E a E z E a E a. E a E z .33... aoN accoe o. ANN.NV m N N N chscsa Daggem Bammmm Avmsswpcoov mewvcmpm pummmsm an mdempm mmmno>¢ .m. manna 140 Table 20; Average Status by Future Orientation FUTURE ORIENTATION Average Pessimistic Status Quo Optimistic Total Row Status (0) (1) (2) N (%D N (%) N (%) N (fi) >fl (Low) 0 1510.7 56 40.0 69 49.3 140 100 1.39 1 1611.8 61 44.8 59 43.4 136 100 1.32 2 13 9.5 59 43.1 65 47.4 137 100 1.38 3 12 7.6 73 46.2 73 46.2 158 100 1.39 4 9 6.2 73 50.0 64 43.8 146 100 1.38 5 9 5.6 64 39.7 88 54.7 161 100 1.49 6 10 5.8 65 37.8 97 56.4 172 100 1.51 7 9 5.8 58 37.2 89 57.0 156 100 1.51 8 6 4.6 47 35.6 79 59.8 132 100 1.55 (High) 9 7 5.3 44 33.1 82 61.6 133 100 1.56 Total 106 ( 7.2) 600 (40.8) 765 (52.0) 1471(100) 1.45 0c1. i 3.71 4.30 4.81 2 I 30.84, at .18, e05>p) .01. Table 211 Status Inconsistency by Powerlessness romamssurss Status (Low) 0 1 (High) 2 Total R_o_w Inconsis. N (79 N (%) N (75) N (7‘) x 53 36.0 51 34.7 43 29.3 147 100 .93 61 38.9 66 42.0 30 19.1 157 100 .80 39 29.5 46 34.9 47 35.6 132 100 1.06 56 36.8 62 40.8 34 22.4 152 100 .86 37 25.1 70 47.6 40 27.2 147 100 1.02 54 34.4 62 39.5 41 26.1 157 100 .92 54 39.4 52 38.0 31 22.6 137 100 .83 50 29.1 75 43.6 47 27.3 172 100; .98 A g O 00-.) came-um... 43 26.4 ‘ 74' 45.4 46 28.2 163 100 1.02 (High) 9 59 39.9) 59 39.9 30 20.2 148 100 .80 Total 506 (33.5) 617 (40.8) 389 (25.7) 1512(100) .92 x2 . 30e3, d: - 18, e05)P) e025e \‘1 n e e s r e K). a c . kl 1.1 a. #1 a. \11 a 141 Table 22: Status Inconsistency by Social Isolation SOCIAL ISOLATION Status (Low) 0 1 (High) 2 Total Hgy Inconsis. N (7.) N (%) N (%) N (7.4) 1 (Low) 0 37 25.0 79 53.4 32 (21.6 148 100 .97 1 37 23.6 81 51.6 39 (24.8 157 100 1.01 2 30 22.6 71 53.4 32 24.0 133 100 1.02 3 34 22.1 89 57.8 31 20.1 154 100 .98 4 37 25.0 74 50.0 37 25.0 148 100 1.00 5 41 25.9 75 47.5 42 26.6 158 100 1.01 6 32 23.2 76 55.1 30 21.7 138 100 .99 7 32 18.5 104 60.1 37 21.4 173 100 1.03 8 44 27.0 94 57.7 25 15.3 163 100 .88 (High) 9 40 27.0 82 55.4 26 17.6 148 100 .91 Total 364 (23.9) 825 (54.3) 331 (21.8) 1520(100) .98 x2 - 15.96, a: - 18, .75>p> .50. Table 23: Status Inconsistency by Normlessness NOHMTHSSNHSS Status (Low) 0 1 (High) 2 Total ng Inconsis. N (76) N (76) N N (7.) x (Low) 0 34 23.1 64 43.6) 49 33.3 147(100) 1.10 1 51 32.7 55 35.3 50 32.0 156 100 .99 2 34 26.2 44 33.8 52 40.0 130 100 1.14 3 28 18.6 68 45.0 55 36.4 151 100 1.18 5 .49 31.0 56 35.5 53 33.5 158 100 1.03 6 36 26.1 56 40.6 46 33.3 138 100 1.07 7 31 ‘709 68 3903 74 42e8 173 100 1e25 8 32 19.8 61 37.6 69 42.6 162 100 1.23 (High) 9 34 23.0 53 35.8 61 41.2 148 100 1.18 Total 356 (23.6) 587 (38.8) 568 (37.6) 1511(100) 1.14 x2 - 28.4, or - 18, .1)p .05. \l \1 1 142 .ON. ANANN. .NN s .3 a e ' NN NN NM .N.N .oo.VeoN. .N.N.V N.N .N.NNV NNN AN.NNV NNN .N.N.V NNN .N.N V 4.. Hs.oe N..N noo.VNN. .N.N.V NN no.NNV Nm Ae.omV Ne .N.N.V NN .N.N.V N. N AnN.NV NN.N noo.VNN. Ao..NV em ...NNV NN .N.NNV NN Am.N.V NN .N.N V N N NN.N .oo.VNN. .N.N.V NN .N.NNV Ne .N.NNV NN .N.N.V Nm .N.N V o. N No.N .oo.VNN. AN.o.V N. .N.NNV Nm AN.NNV .N .N.0NV NN .N.N V N. N c..N Aoo.VNN. ...N.V .N .N.NNV Nm AN..NV oN N..NNV NM AN.N V e. N NN.N noo.VNe. .N.N.V NN .N.NNV Ne .o.emV 0N .N...V N. N..N V N e NN.N .oo.VON. .N.N V m. NN.NNV NN .N.NNV Ne .o.N.V NN .N.N V N N NN.N Aoo.VNN. .N.N.V N. .N.NNV NN .N.NNV NN .N.NNV NN .N.N V N N NN.. Aoo.VNN. NN.N.V N. .N.NNV Nm .N.NNV NN .N..NV em .N.N.V N. . N..N noo.VNe. AN.N.V NN Ao.eNV NN .N.NNV Ne .N.N.V NN AN.N V e. o AsoaV M E a E a E a E z E z E z .3288 SON anooe N ANNHNV N N . o Aaqu unsc.m nmmzmmfisNozummmmefimmeoa mmosmmoassomlmmommmOHsosom Mp homepmwmsocsH mapspm .VN canoe . 143 NN.NsocV .NN. «naoN. ...N s be .NN.NN ... Ne NN.N.V NNN .N.N V NN .N.N V NN .N.o N. .N.o V N. “0.. V N. Hcsca Ao.N.V NN NN.N V .. .e.. V N No.0 V 0 No.0 V o Ae.. V N N AsmamV N..NNV NN .N.N V N .N.N V N .N.. V N .N.o V . AN.. V N N .N.N.V NN N..N V N .N.N V N No.0 V o AN.. V N NN.. V N N NN.N.V NN .N.N V o. .N.o V . AN.. V N AN.o V . .N.N V N N NN.NNV NN NN.N V N “N.N V 4 No.0 V o no.0 V o “N.o V . N NN.N.V NN NN.N V N. Ao.N V N .o.o V o .N.o V . AN.o V . e ...NNV NN NN.N V N NN.N V e NN.o V . “N.o V . AN.o V . N .N..NV NN NN.N V N Ao.N V e .N.N V N .N.N V N No.0 V N N AN...V N. NN.N V e .N.N V N AN.o V . .N.. V N .N.o V . . NN.N.V NN .N.N V .. Ao.N V N .N.. V N NN.o V . Ao.N V o 0 Asqu E a E z E z E z E z E a .8888 N e N N . o AscaV anscsn 02H9249m BBHMMQM. msaussvm «demean an homepagesoosH unseen .mm capes 144 NN.N .oo.Vo.N. NN.N.V NNN N..N V No. “N.N.V NNN NN.N.V NNN ...N.V N.N Ncsoe oo.N Aoo.VNN. .N.N.V NN NN.N.V N. .N.N.V N. no.N.V NN .N.N.V N. N ANNNNV 0N.N .oo.VON. .N.N.V NN .N.N V N. NN.N.V NN .N.N.V NN AN.o.V N. N 0N.N Aoo.VNN. N..N.V NN AN.N V .. NN.N.V NN Ao.N.V .N .N.N.V NN N 0N.N Aoo.VNN. ...N.V N. NN.N V N AN..NV NN .N.N V N. .N.N.V NN N NN.N Aoo.VNN. NN.N.V NN Ao.N V .. NN.N.V NN NN.N.V NN AN.N.V 0N N NN.N Aoo.VNN. .N.N.V N. A..N V N. .N.N.V NN .N.N.V 0N .N.N.V NN N ON.N Aoo.VNN. AN.N.V NN .N.N V N .N.N.V 0N .N.N.V NN .N.N.V NN N 0N.N Aoo.VNN. “N.N.V N. .N.N V N NN.N.V NN Ao.N.V 0N “N.N.V N. N o..N Aoo.VNN. NN.N.V NN .N.N V o. NN.NNV NN NN.N.V NN NN.N.V NN . 0N.N Aoo.VNN. AN...V N. NN.N V N NN.N.V NN .N.N.V NN .N...V N. o .aqu M. E z E z E a E z E z E a .3888. aoN Nasoe o. ANN.NV N N N N n:.c.m UZHQZdEm BZMmEmm AnceszsooV mewcsspm pcomonm Mp NosONNNNSOOOH msvspm .mm manna 145 Table 261 Status Inconsistency by Future Orientation FUTURE ORIENTATION Status Pessimistic Status Que Optimistic Total Row Inconsis. (O) (1) (2) N N (96) N (9%) N (76) 3? A (Low) 0 12 8.6) 50 36.0 77 55.4 139 100 1.47 1 13 8.4 67 43.5 74 48.1 154 100 1.40 2 6 4.6 48 36.9 76 58.5 130 100 1.45 3 9 6.0 65 43.3 76 50.7 150 100 1.45 4 10 6.8 55 37.7 81 55.5 146 100 1.49 5 9 5.8 63 40.9 82 53.3 154 100 1.47 6 8 5.9 51 37.8 76 56.3 135 100 1.50 7 14 8.3 66 39.3 88 52.4 168 100 1.44 8 15 9.7 77 49.7 63 40.6 155 100 1.31 (High) 9 10 7.2 58 41.4 72 51.4 140 100 1.44 Total 106 ( 7.2) 600 (40.8) 765 (42.0) 1471(100) 1.45 x2 . 16.70, or -18, .75)p 3.50. utilizing controls. As background, then, we shall use the full ten-value range Of Status Inconsistency in examining these relationships. Only one Of these tables contains a statistically significant relationship (Table 21), and none contain a patterning of rela- tionships which would allow any theoretical interpretation. There are few differences in the row means as one inspects the various levels of SI. Generally, it would appear that SI has little effect upon alienation. ‘We would argue, however, that this is due to the confounding effect Of Average Status, and that in the tables relating SI to alienation, while controlling for the effect of AS, a significant relationship will obtain. Relationship Of Social Mebility to Dependent Variables Tables 27-32 detail the relationship Of Social Mobility to alienation, and to Present Standing and Future Orientation. An 146 Table 27: Social Mobility by Powerlessness POWERLESSNESS Social (low) 0 1 (High) 2 Total 32w Mobility N (%) N (%) N (%) N (%) X Down 52 (43.0) 48 (39.7) 21 (17.3) 121 (100) .74 Stable 294 (30.9) 392 (41.2) 266 (27.9) 952 (100) .97 Up 127 (38.6) 132 (40.1) 70 (21.3) 329 (100) .83 Total 473 (33.7) 573 (40.8) 357 (25.5) 1402 (100) .92 x? . 6.08, df - 4, .25) p) .10. Table 28: Social Mobility by Social Isolation SOCIAL ISOLATION Social (Low) 0 1 (High) 2 Total 29w Mobility N (76) N (73) N (91») N (’4‘) X Down 28 (23.0) 73 (59.8) 21 (17.2) 122 (100) .94 Stable 226 (23.6) 523 (54.6) 209 (21.8) 958 (100) .98 Up 91 (27.7) 160 (48.6) 78 (23.7) 329 (100) .96 Total 345 (24.5) 756 (53.7) 308 (21.8) 1409 (100) .97 x2 - 6.08, or - 4, .25>p 5.10. Table 291 Social Mobility by Normlessness NORMLESSNESS Social (low) 0 1 (High) 2 Total ng Nobility N (%) N (%) N (%) N (%) x Down 29 (24.0) 54 (44.6) 38 (31.4) 121 (100) 1.07 Stable 205 (21.5) 366 (38.4) 382 (40.1) 953 (100) 1.19 Up 101 (30.7) 127 (38.6) 101 (30.7) 329 (100) 1.00 Total 335 (23.9) 547 (39.0) 521 (37.1) 1403 (100) 1.13 x2 .. 16.7, df - 4, .OOS)p).OO1. 147 Q ..oo. ANANoo. .N .. ..e .NN.NN .. Ne oN.N Aoo.V NNN. NN.N.V NN. NN.NNV NNN A..NNV NNN .N.N.V .NN NN.N V No. fleece NN.N Aoo.V NNN .N.N V NN NN.NNV NN No..NV No. “N..NV NN .N.N V NN as NN.N Aoo.V NNN NN.N.V NN. NN.NNV .NN .N.NNV oNN ...N.V .N. NN.N V oN cancen No.N .oo.V oN. .N.o.V N. NN.NNV NN .N.NNV 0N .N.NNV NN AN.N V .. neon M E a E e E e E e E e E a has; pom Naece N ANN.NV N N . 0 Asqu NsNoom mmazequnmozlnmszmmnqmmsoa mmeemueNshOZTOueenueNaesem he MPNNNnoa Neuoom .om canoe 148 .NN. AN SN. .8 u we .N.NN .. Na .oo.V NN.N.V NN.NV NN.N.V NN.N.V .N.N.V NN.N.V NN.NV .N.NV .N.oV AN.oV AN.oV NN.N ooN. NoN .o. NNN N.N NoN NNN NN NN o. .. N. fleece .oo.V Ao.N.V NN.NV ANN N.V .NN N.V NN.N.V .N.N.V NN.NV AN. NNV ANN oV AN. .oV .N.oV No.N NNN NN .N NN . as Aoo.V .N.N.V ...NV NN. N.V .N. N.V NN. N.V .N. N.V Ac. NV NN.NV AN. oV “ON .V A...V NN.N oNN NN. NN NN. NN. NN. NN. NN NN N o. cashew .oo.V AN...V NN.NV NN.N.V NN.N.V ...N.V NN.NNV AN.oV NN..V Ao.oV .N.oV .N..V NN.N .N. N. N. N. NN N. .N . N o . N neon M E e E2 rm: EN EN EN E2 E2 Es Es En E e 3:32. son fleece o. N N N N N N N N . o Nc.oom ANNNNV oanzNNm Nznmnmm .aqu msNNsseN escnchm an N..NNpos Hc.oom ..N canoe 149 Table 32: Social Mobility by Future Orientation FUTURE ORIENTATION Social (Low) 0 1 (High) 2 Total ng Nobility N (70) N 0%) N (%) N (%> x Down 9 7.4 62 é51.3 so é41.3) 121 100) 1.34 Stable 71 7.7 369 39.9 485 52.4; 925 100) 1.45 Up 19 6.0 127 (39.9 172 (54.1 318 100) 1.48 Total 99 (7.3) 558 (40.9) 707 (51.8) 1364 (100) 1.45 x2 .- 7.37, at - 4, .05>p> .10. inspection of these tables reveals that SM is significantly related to P (Table 27), N (Table 29) and to their combination PN (Table 30), but not to SOI (Table 28). Those statistics which are significant have probabilities of .005 or less. In these three significant tables, it would appear that the "Stable" group is in all cases the most alienated, in that this group has the highest row means-or mean alienation. And, in all cases, there is less difference between the "Up" and "Down" groups than between the "Stables” and any other group. This finding partially supports those of Meier and Bell in that the "Up" SM group is less alienated than the "Stable" group. Our finding that the "Stable" group is the most alienated, however, contradicts Meier and Bell's findings.2 Another interpretation of this set of tables, though, is that Average Status is confounding the relationship of Social Mobility to alienation. That is, from Table 2 we note that the "Stable" group had the lowest AS, and the "Up" SM group has the highest status. These notions, coupled with the previous finding of a relationship between AS and alienation, may account for the 150 relationship of Social Mbbility to alienation. If this is the case, then we should expect that this relationship will dis- appear when a control for AS is introduced into the relationship of SM and alienation. We would also comment on the lack of a relationship between Social Isolation and Social Mobility. If this relationship is, in fact, confounded by AS, then we may be seeing again the lack of a relationship between AS and SOI. Tables 31 and 32 illustrate the relationship between SM, and PS and F0 (or better, the lack of a relationship because neither of these tables develops a significant chi square. With respect to Table 31, however, we might notice that the PS means for each category of SM do, in fact, correspond to the order which would have been predicted by referring to the AS mean for each SM group. That is, the ”Stable" group has the lowest AS mean and the lowest PS mean; and the "Up" SM group has the highest AS mean and the highest PS mean. With respect to Table 32, however, this is not the case-the F0 means do.ggt_folloW’the order that would have been predicted by referring to AS means. In summary, it would appear that SM is related to alienation, but that this relationship may be accounted for by reference to the AS contamination of each level or category of SM. If this is in fact true, then these relationships would disappear when controlled for AS; if there is any true relationship of SM to alienation, then it should remain when the AS control is intro- ducedo 151 Second Order Controlled Relationships Average Status Relationships In this section, we shall examine the relationship of Average Status to alienation, etc., while controlling for Social Mobility and Status Inconsistency. In these controls, as well as the remaining controls inthis chapter, the values of AS and SI are collapsed from the ten-value range presented in previous tables, to five- and three-value ranges, respectively. Again, this is done in order to keep the number of cells in the over-all tables within an interpretable range, and also to assure suf- ficient N in each cell to allow legitimate interpretation. Tables 33-38 describe the relationship of AS to the dependent variables while controlling for SM, and Tables 39-44 describe the relationship of AS to the dependent variables while controlling for SI. Tables 33, 35 and 36 indicate that the original relationship of AS to P, N and PN is not disrupted by the control for SM. In each case, there is a generally inverse linear regression of alienation means (row means) on A5, for each level of SM. The only consistent exception to this pattern occurs in the "Up" control category, where at the second level of AS, the row mean is higher than one would expect-there is an increase in the mean rather than a decrease consistent with the general linear regression. we would take these patterns as confirming a relationship between AS and alienation. Table 34 documents the lack of a relationship between AS and Social Isolation. This corresponds to the original finding in 152 Table 33: Average Status by Powerlessness at Each Level of Social Mobility Social POWERLESSNESS Mobility Average (101120 1 (High) 2 Total R21! Control Status N %) N (%) N (9‘) N (7°) X (Lo) 0 4 30.8 6 46.1 3 23.1 13 100) .92 1 12 40.0 11 36.7 7 23.3 30 100g .83 Down 2 14 42.4 14 42.4 5 15.2 33 100 .73 3 17 50.0 12 35.3; 5E14.7 34 100; .65 (Hi) 4 5 45.5 5 45.5 1 9.1 11 100 .64 (Lo) 0 51E21.8 76 32.5; 107 45.7 234 100 1.24 1 60 27.4 103 47.0 56 25.6 219 100 .98 Stable 2 61 32.6 81 43.3 45 24.1 187 100 .91 3 67 38.1 69 39.2 40 22.7 176 100 .85 (Hi) 4 55 40.4 63 46.3 18 13.3 136 100 .73 (Lo) 0 3 27.3 5 45.4 3 27.3 11 100) 1.00 1 6 20.0 14 46.7 10 33.3 30 100 1.13 Up 2 19 27.5 33 47.8 17 24.7 69 100 .97 3 48 45.3 38 35.8 20 18.9 106 100 .74 (Hi) 4 51 45.1 42 37.2 20 17.7 113 100 .73 Table 34: Average Status by Social Isolation at Each Level of Social Mobility Social ( ) socle ISOLATI?N ) Mobility Average me 0 1 High 2 Total 3gp Control Status N (70 N N (9%) N (3%) x (Lo) 0 3 23.1 753. 8 3 23.1 13 100 1.00 1 4 12.9167 7 6 19.4 31 100 1.06 Down 2 10 30.3 957 6 4 12.1 33 100 .82 3 8 25.5161.8 5 14.7 34 100 .91 (Hi) 4 . 3 27.3545 4 3 27.3 11 100 1.00 (Lo) 0 36 15.2 137 58. 3) 62 26.4) 235 100 1.11 1 51 23.1 122 55. 2 48 21.7 221 100 .99 Stable 2 57 30.3148.4 40 21.3 188 100 .91 3 49 27.7 252.0 36 20.3 177 100 .93 (H1) 4 33 24.1159.1 23 16.8 137 100 .93 (Lo) 0 32 7.3 6 54.5 2 18.2 11 100 .91 1 103 3.3 653 3 4 13.4 30 100 .80 Up 2 1217.4 652.2 21 30.4 69 100 1.13 3 292 “.4? b§45 3; 29 27.3 106 100; 1.00 (Hi) 4 373 2.7 447 8 22 19.5 113 100 .87 153 mm. 00. m.. n.0m 0m e.mv me ..0m em 0 ..mv mm. 00. 00. 0.mm em 0.0m mm 0.0e me n N... 00. mm ...m 0m 0.0m mw ..0m 0. m 00 >0.. 00. on e.gn p. m.mm 0. 0.0. m . 00.. 00. .. n.0m e n.0m e v..~ m 0 .600 mm. 00. pm. N..N mm ..0e 00 ..mm we 0 Aemv em. 00. ... ...N me 0.0m mm m.mm mm m m... 00. 00. 0.0m 00 e..e .. 0.- .e N napepm mm.. 00. .NN 0.0e mm m.mm 0. 0.0. we . mm.. 00. «mm ..mm .m. 0.mm 0. m.. .. o .600 00.. 00. .. m..m m e.me m m..m m e Aemv 00.. 00. em e.m~ 0. m..e e. «.mm 0. m 00.. 00. mm N..N . 0.0m 0. m.em 0 N neon .m. 00. .m ..0m m. ..0m m. 0.N~ . . 00.. 00. m. 0.0m 0 ...e m «.0 . 0 .600 m E z 03 e g z E 2 8:3 33:8 gem fleece m Aemfimv . 0 .3800 emceeee a»...pes He.eem unonmmOHSHOz hpaaapos Hofioom mo Hoboq noun pd uncannoaanoz hp usvdpm owdnobq .mn canoe 154 00.. .00. 0.. ...N 0 0.0N 00 N..0 Ne 0.0N .N ..m .. e “.00 0... 00. 00. 0.0 0 0.0N 0N 0.00 00 0..N 0N 0.N. 0. 0 NN.N 00. 00 ..0 0 ..00 .N ..0N 0. v... N. ..0 0 N 00 .0.N 00. 00 0.00 0. A0.0¢ N. 0.0. 0 0.0. 0 0.0 . . 00.N 00.V .. .0.0. N .0000 e 0.00 e 0.0 0 ..m . 0 .600 .0.. M00. 00. ..0 0 ..0. 0N 0.00 00 0.0N 00 0.0 N. e ..00 00.. 00. 0.. 0.. 0. 0..N 00 0.00 0. 0..N 00 ..0 0. 0 NN.N 00. 00. 0.0. 0N 0.0N 00 0.00 00 0... N0 0.. 0. N 6.00.0 0N.N 00. 0.N 0.0. «0 0.0N .0 0.N0 .. 0... 00 0.0 0. . N0.N 00. .0N m.N0 0. 0.00 0. 0.0N 00 0.0. 0N ... 0 0 .600 00.N 00. .. ..m . .0..N 0 0..N 0 0.00 e 0.0 0 0 ..00 m... 00. 00 0.0 0 ..0. m 0.00 0. 0.0N 0 ..0. m 0 .m.. 00. 00 ..m 0 N.0. 0 «.Ne e. 0.00 0. 0.0 . N 0560 o..N 00. 00 ..0. 0 ..0N 0 N.0N . ..0. 0 ..0. 0 . .0.N 00. N. 0.0 . 0.00 . 0.0N 0 0.0 . 0.0 0 0 .600 N 0 Z 0 z 0 Z 0 Z 0 z o z mflvdam HOHuhoo 000 meoe w.~00.0v ..00 AEN “.0 . .mVAeeAV omene>< 00.00062 00.660 mmMmeHHEMOZIMmHmeQHmmBDm 09.3302 Hufioom Ho 02.3 noum pd mmocmmoasnohelmmonnmoanokom .00. magnum omunobd .mm 000.09 Table 371 Average Status by Present Standing 155 at Each Level of Social Mobility* Social MObility Average Mean Control Status Present Standing (Lo) 0 5.77 1 6.61 Down 2 6.63 3 7.24 (Hi) 4 7.64 (La) 0 6.52 1 6.57 Stable 2 6.67 . 3 7-19 (Hi) 4 7.13 (Lo) 0 6.73 1 6.40 Up 2 7.10 3 7.17 (Hi) 4 7.40 * Because of the extreme length of the full table, only the means are presented here. Table 381 Average Status by Future Orientation at Each Level of Social Mobility Social FUTURE ORIENTATION Mobility Average Pessimistic Status Optimistic Total Row Control Status (0) Quoé1g (2) __ N (76) N 76 N (96) N (76) J! (Lo) 0 7.7 8 61.5) 4 30.8 13 100 1.23 1 16.1 12 38.7 14 45.2 31 100 1.29 Down 2 6.2 19 59.4 11 34.4 32 100 1.28 3 2.9 19 55.9 14 41.2 34 100 1.38 (Hi) 4 0.0 4 36.4 7 63.6 11 100 1.64 (Lo) 0 10.7 90 40.2 110(49.1) 224 100)1.38 1 8.9 96 45.1 98 46.0 213 100 1.37 Stable 2 4.9 79 42.9 96 52.2 184 100 1.47 3 7.0 59 34.3 101 58.7 172 100 1.52 (Hi) 4 5.3 45 34.1 80 60.6 1132 100 1.55 (Lo) 0 0.0 7 70.0 3 30.0 10 100 1.30 1 3.9 14 53.8 11 42.3 26 100 1.38 3 5.7 41 38.7 59 55.6 106 100 1.50 (Hi) 4 5.4) 36 32.4 69 62.2 111 100 1.57 156 Table 391 Average Status by Pewerlessness at Each Level of Status Inconsistency St POWERLESSNESS atus Inconsis. Average (Law)0 W1 (High) 2 Total ng Control Status N (70) N (%) 1106) x 0 22 16.8 030. 5 69 52.7 131 100 1.36 1 10 31.2 546.9 7 21.9 32 100 .91 (low) 2 16 42.1 36. 8 8 21.1 38 100 .79 0 3 37 41.1 640. 0 17 18.9 90 100 .78 4 68 46.9 840. 0 19 13.1 145 100 .66 0 23 30.7 925 3 33 44.0 75 100 1.13 1 32 25.8 645 2 36 29.0 124 100 1.03 1 2 48 29.8 049- 7 33 20.5 161 100 .91 3 61 43.3934 7 31 22.0 141 100 .79 4 37 40.2 245- 7 13 14.1 92 100 .74 0 18 21.9 36 43.9 28 34.2 82 100 1.12 1 41 27.3 70 46.7 39 26.0 150 100 .99 (High) 2 36 31.0 50 43.1 30 25.9 116 100 .95 2 3 45 44.6 38 37.6 18 17.8 101 100 .73 4 12 35.3 14 41.2 8 23.5 34 100 .88 Table 40: Average Status by Social Isolation at Each Level of Status Inconsistency SOCIAL ISOLATION (Low) 0 1 (High) 2 Total Row N (70 N (90 N (7") N(%) X 40 30.3 Status Inconsis. Control 132 100 1.16 7 21.915 46.9 10 31.2 32 100 1.09 (Low) 10 26.3 257.9 6 15.8 38 100 .89 0 31 34.4 43 47. 8 16 17.8 90 100 .83 31 21.2 146 100 .96 56 6 20 26.36100 57. 27 21.4 126 100 1. 00 49. 40 24.7 162 100 .99 E4 g ; 1422100; .99 5 100 .91 2100 .99 .97 .91 .96 .76 100 100 100 4100 151 116 (High) 2 101 A #UN-IO #WN-to #041me «hm-b 10.11» MAM/WWW —b a.) O O O 4:... WWWVWVW (Dal-P- omw 157 Pm. Aoow 4m Am.mmv m 0.0mv a, m.mmv a 4 6m. Moor we? >.wm mm m.mm pm P.4m mm m m mc.. 00? ma? r.mm m4 m.o¢ av 0.0m mm m Asmflmv mm.v Moor In, 0.64 45 e.>m on m.mr cm a mv.P 00? mm m.mm m4 m.om mm o.Fr m 0 am. oowv mm m.NNW Fm m.r¢ mm m.mmw mm 8 mm. 004 m4? m.~m mm n.44 mm 0.4m 44 m NP.F 00. cm. 4.6mM mm m.mv ow «.NNM mm N F mm.c 004 mm. 8.86 mm m.~m we m.mF mm a Pm.v cop fir m.wm m4 w.~m mm v.m v o um. 00? 84? 8.4m mm m.~v mm F.mu .8 8 mp. cow om o.om m? m.~m 4m m.~¢ mm m 0 mm. co, mm m.mm or m.mm e. m.mm v? m Agogv 4m. ooFW Pm Mm.mmw m o.mm m m.mv 4. F No.4 00. mm? >..m m» m.wm pm 6.6 V N? o m E 2 g a g z 93 z 8436 3:68 30m Hmpoe m hamfimv F o ABQHV omdno>d .mfimsoosH mspupm mmmzmmmqsmoz hosopmwmcoosH unpopm mo Hoboq soda as mmosmmoasnoz an mapdwm omsuo>4 ..w canoe 158 Fm.F ooFW4m m.m F m.mm m N.F4 4F n.4m a Am.m m 4 mm.F ooF FoF m.m oF m.mF 4F 4.mm mm F.m~ on m.m m m m NN.N ooF mFF o.mF mF 4.mm 4m 4.Fm mm m.mF Fm 4.4 m N Asmfimv mm.m 00F 00F 0.0m on o.mm m4 4.4m 44 c.4F 4N 4.4 oF F mm.m ooF mm N.4m mm 4.mm FN 4.Fm 4m «.4 4 4.m m o mm.F ooF mm m.4 4 4.4m mm 4.mm on «.NN mm o.~F FF 4 mm.F ooF F4F 4.m w n.4m pm 4.4m 4m 4.mF 4m m.m 4F 4 4m.m 00F 04F 4.4F mm 4.4m mm m.Fm om F.wF mm 4.m m N F mm.m ooF 4NF m.mF mF n.mm 44 4.44 4m m.mF mF m.4 4 F OF.N 00F 44 o.mm 4F 4.4m mm 0.4m om m.oF m 4.F F o 44.F ooF m4F 4.m m P.0N cm F.m4 F4 «.4m mm 4.4 FF 4 F~.F ooF om n.4 4 o.om mF 4.4m Fm 4.4m mm 4.4F mF m o mm.F 00F 44 m.m m 4.Fm NF 4.4m FF o.Fm m «.mF m N ésQHV om.F ooF Fm m.NF 4 m.mF 4 4.44 NF m.mm 4 m.~F 4 F mm.m ooF FNF 4.mm m4 F.4m N4 F.mF mm o.FF 4F A4.N m o m 33 2 as 2 as 4 as 4 44v 2 443 z .334. 8.348 30m Hayes 4 Asmflmv m w w o Agoqv omdnobd .mamsoocH mspupm mmmzwmmaamoznmmmzmmmqmmsom hosopmfimsoosH nspwpm mo Ho>on scum pd moosmmoHaHoZImmosunoanotpm hp napopm omdnobd .Nv canoe 159 Table 431 Average Status by Present Standing at all Levels of Status Inconsistency Status Inconsis. Control Average Mean Status PRESENT STANDING ” (Low) 0 (High) 2 #wm-so #UJN-fio #UN-‘O 6.44 6.03 6.71 7.18 7.21 6.37 6.54 6.80 7.16 6.85 6.73 6.54 6.90 6.72 7.44 Table 44: Average Status by Future Orientation at Each Level of Status Inconsistency FUTURE ORIENTATION Status Average Pessimistic Status Optimistic Total Row Inconsis. Status (0) QuoE1g (2) ._ Control N (%) N 94 N (7.) N(%) x 0 16 12.7) 51%40.5 59(46.8 126 100; 1.34 1 2 6.7 13 43.3 15 50.0 39 100 1.43 (Low) 2 1 2.61 19 50.0 18 47.4 38 100) 1.45 0 3 5 5.7 27530.7 56 63.6 88 100; 1.58 4 7 5.0) 55 39.0 79 56.0 141 100 1.51 0 6g 8.3) 32(44 5) 34(47.2) 72(100) 1.39 1 9 7'31 53é43.1; 61149.6; 123E100? 1.42 1 2 9 5.7 66 41.5 84 52.8 159 100 1.47 3 8E 5.7g 60é42.9; 72é51.43 140E1003 1.46 4 4 4.4 23 25 3 64 70.3 91 100 1.66 o 9 11.5 34(43. 6 35 44.9) 78 100) 1.33 1 14 9.9 66 £46. 5 62 43.6 142 100 1.34 (High) 2 8 7.3 52 47 3 50 45.4: 110 100; 1.38 2 3 6 6.0 364 36. 0 58 58.0 100 100 1.52 4 2( 6.1) 13(39 4) 18(54.5) 33(100) 1.48 160 Table 16, and again indicates the possible separability of SOI as a component of alienation, with respect to AS. Table 37 depicts the relationship of AS and Present Standing. In this table, we have presented only the mean PS for each AS level, at each level of SM, rather than the full eleven-value PS range. This was done because the original table with all eleven values contained 154 cells, and.with some cells contain- ing few or no N, making the full table of doubtful value. In any case, inspection of the means allows one to make sufficient interpretations. Inspection of these means reveals patterns which are similar to those presented in Table 19. Generally, there is an increase in PS means with each increase in AS level, at each level of SM. The only exception occurs at the second AS level in the "Up" SM category, where the PS mean dr0ps below that which one would have expected based upon the general linear increase. Generally, the relationships are much more "linear" than in the original table. Table 38 characterizes the relationship between AS and Future Orientation. Again, there is a generally linear rela- tionship, with each AS level having a higher F0 mean, and a higher percentage of persons in the "Optimistic" category than any lower AS level. This relationship is present at each level of the SM.control. we would take this as evidence that AS is related to F0, irrespective of any effect that SM might have upon F0. Tables 39, 41 and 42 indicate that AS is related.to P, N and PN, at each level of control for Status Inconsistency. Again, 161 each level of AS has a lower alienation mean than the next higher AS level, and the inverse linear relationship remains. Table 40 indicates that there is a general inverse linear regression for SOI, but the magnitude of the differences between the various SOI means are very small, which would lead us to state that again, there is no relationship between AS and SOI. Table 43 indicates that AS is related to PS, at each level of control for SI. Again, the relationship is linear and direct. The only exceptions are at the high and low consistency levels, where the relationship is slightly curvilinear, with the lowest AS level having a higher PS mean than the next highest AS level. Given the general linearity of the rest of the PS means, however, we would interpret this as a linear relationship. Table 44 indicates a direct relationship between A8 and F0, at each level of SI. Generally, there are higher percentages of persons in the "Optimistic" category at the higher AS levels, than at the lower AS levels, at each level of SI. We would interpret this to mean that AS is related to F0, irrespective of any influence that SI might exert. In summary, it would appear that Average Status is consistently related to alienation, as indexed by the alienation sub-scales—- as well as by Present Standing and Future 0rientation—-and that the effect of SI and SM on AS is little or none. we would also note that 501 seems to be somewhat independent index of aliena- tion, at least as far as AS is concerned. It may be that SOI indexes a form of alienation that is characteristic of some other more or less "specialized" population, but it is definitely not r, 162 characteristic of high or low Average Status. We might note that the control for SM—-at least at the "Up" category——tends to produce a deflection in the linear regression of alienation means at the second, or lower-middle, status level. This is also the level at which Status Inconsistency "peaks" in its relationship to Average Status, so that perhaps the Social Mobility control allows SI to exert its effect in selected places upon the AS scale. This may be borne out in examining later tables depicting the relationship of SI to alienation, while controlling for AS and SM simultaneously. Status Inconsistency Relationships Tables 45—50 characterize the relationship of SI to alienation, at each level of SM. An inspection of these tables reveals, briefly, that there is no consistent pattern, and therefore, absolutely no relationship between SI and alienation. The only possible exception is the Table 48, where a linear, direct rela- tionship between SI and PN appears. Examination of Tables 45 and 47 (for P and N, separately), however, reveals that neither of these component tables indicate a linear relationship; further, they appear to create an impression of linearity due to mutual canceling out of differences. That is, when Tables 45 and 47—- which make up Table 48—-are taken separately, they indicate no consistent relationship; when they are fused as they are in Table 48, it would appear that they spuriously create an impres- sion of linearity. we would argue that all three tables must have a consistent linear pattern to constitute "proof" of a Table Social Eobilit Control I! Ibwn Stable W Tabl 80c1 1101211 Cont: DQWI Stab: 163 Table 451 Status Inconsistency by Powerlessness at Each Level of Social Mobility POWERLESSNESS Social Mobility Status (Low) 0 1 (High) 2 Total ng Control Inconsis. N (%) N (%) N (%) N (%) X (Lo) 0 15 44.1 17 50.0 2 5.9 34 100 .62 Down 1 27 51.9 16 30.8 9 17.3 52 100 .65 (Hi) 2 10 28.5 15 42.9 10 28.6 35 100 1.00 (Lo) 0 95 33.7) 100 35.5 87 30.8 282 100) .97 Stable 1 106 29.0) 163 44.7 96 26.3 365 100; .97 (Hi) 2 93 30.5 129 42.3 83 27.2 305 100 .97 (Lo) 0 33 41.8 32 40.5 14 17.7 79 100) .76 Up 1 49 39.2 45 36.0 31 24.8 125 100; .86 (Hi) 2 45 36.0 55 44.0 25 20.0 125 100 .84 Table 46: Status Inconsistency by Social Isolation at Each Level of Social Mobility Social SOCIAL ISOLATION Mobility Status (Low) 0 1 (High) 2 Total ‘Bpw Control Inconsis. N (93) N (7%) N W?) N X (Lo) 0 9 26.5 20 58.8 5 14.7 34 100 .88 Down 1 12 22.6 29 54.7 12 22.7 53 100 1.00 (Hi) 2 7 20.0 24 68.6 4 11.4 35 100 .91 (Lo) 0 68 23.9 144 50.7 72 25.4 284 100 1.01 Stable 1 89 24.2 195 53.0 84 22.8 368 100 .99 (Hi) 2 69 22.6 184 60.1 53 17.3 306 100 .95 (Lo) 0 22 27.8 39 49.4 18 22.8 79 100 .95 Up 1 32 25.6 60 48.0 33 26.4 125 100 1.01 (Hi) 2 37 29.6 61 48.8 27 21.6 125 100 .92 soaaanos Hoaoom so Ho>oq some so mmocomoassoz be bozoFmchoosH asaosm amt oases 164 No.F Foon mNF A4.mmV N4 FN.mmv 44 AN.FmV mm N Famv mm. Foon mNF AN.NNV 44 F4.F4v Nm F4.mNV Fm F as am. Foon ma AF.va mN AN.mmV Fm A~.F4v mN o Foqv Nm.F Foon mom AN.~4V 44F A4.44v 4FF F4.va 44 N FFmv NF.F Foon 444 F4.mmV F4F Fm.mmV 44F Am.FNv ow F ospaFm ao.F FooFV FmN Fm.4mv 4m F4.~mV 40F Am.4NV m» o Fonv 40.F Foon mm F4.NNV oF F4.m4v 4F FN.NNV m N Famv NF.F FooFV Nm A4.4MV NF Fm.N4v NN FF.va NF F asap mo.F Foon 4m F4.va oF AF.44V mF Am.4NV m o Foqv m as a S a S a as a .3384 2.28 36m Haaoa N Aswamv F o Agony asFaFm A sPFMFnos as com mmmzmmmnsmoz hpfiafinoz aswoom no Ho>oq seem as unosuuoaanoz hp hosopmwmsoosH nspupm .>¢ canoe huwdflnofi Hsfioom ho Herod «Foam 90 mmmcmm®HEH021mm02mmmfihmkonw Nan koflmpmflmmoonflu mswmfim. um“. mNnmau 165 mo.N FooFV 4NF Fo.NFV 4F Ao.4NV 44 FN.FNV 4m A4.NNV 4N F4.oFV «F N FFmV mo.N FooFV mNF A4.m V F “4.44V m4 F4.mNV Fm Fo.ONV 4N F4.oFV 4F F a: 4m.F FooFV NF F4.m V m Fm.mNV 4N FN.mmV Fm AF.4NV NF A4.F V 4 o FogV 4m.N FooFV 4cm A4.mFV 04 Ao.mNV 4F AN.N4V mm “4.FFV mm F4.m V FF N AamV NN.N AooFV 44m Ao.4FV Fm FF.FNV FoF Fo.4mV FmF Fm.FFV m4 Ao.m V 4F F onaFm FF.N FooFV mFN A4.4FV 44 Fm.NNV 44 A4.NmV Fm AF.mFV mm Fo.m V 4N o AQNV mN.N FooFV mm Fm.4FV m Am.NNV 4 Fo.04V 4F AF.FFV 4 FF.4 V N N FFmV oo.N FooFV F4 F4.FFV 4 Am.FNV FF F4.mmV FF F4.FNV FF A4.FFV 4 F neon 44.F FooFV 4m Fm.m V N Fm.4NV m Fm.4NV m Fm.NmV FF F4.4 V m o FonV 44 44V 2 $3 a 44V 2 FeV 2 as 4 FeV zmlmataooshw F9348 tom FaFoe 4 FsNFmV m N F o AsoaV asaapm sFaFFpos mmmzmmsqsmoznmmmammmqmmaom 7 Hoaoom hpaflpos stoom Mo H243 soon as noosmnoHaHozlunosuuoHsotom ho. honopnansoofi” napsvm .24 39.49 H -l 166 Table 49: Status Inconsistency by Present Standing at Each Level of Social Mobility Social Mobility Status Mean Control Inconsis. PRESENT STANDING (Lo) 0 7.09 Down 1 6051 (Hi) 2 6.94 (Lo) 0 6.76 Stable 1 6.72 (Hi) 2 6.84 U4)0 7J6 Up 1 7.14 (Hi) 2 6.82 Table 50: Status Inconsistency by Future Orientation at Each Level of Social Mobility FUTURE ORIENTATION Social Status Pessimistic Status Optimistic Total Row Nobility Inconsis. (0) Quo g (2) ._ Control N (%) N V N (%) N C%) X (Lo) 0 1( 2.9) . 19(55.9) 14(41.2) 34(100) 1.38 Down 1 2( 3.8) 27(50.9) 24(45.3) 53(100) 1.42 (Hi) 2 6(17.6) 16(47.1) 12(35.3) 34(100) 1.18 (Lo) 0 23( 8.4) 106(39.0) 143(52.6) 272(100) 1.44 Stable 1 26( 7.3) 139(38.8) 193(53.9) 358(100) 1.47 (Hi) 2 22( 7.5) 124(42.o) 149(5o.5) 295(100) 1.43 (Lo) 0 3( 3.9) 26(38.3) 49(62.8) 78(100) 1.59 Up 1 7( 5.7) 50(41.0) 65(53.3) 122(100) 1.48 (Hi) 2 9( 7.6) .51(43.2) 58(49.2) 118(100) 1.42 .a 7 n T e .2 and I mus 167 relationship. And they do not. Tables 51—56 detail the relationship of SI to alienation, when controlling for AS. According to our original hypotheses, AS confounds the expected relationship of SI to alienation. Therefore, in this set of tables, there should be a relationship between SI and alienation. But, as with the previous tables, there are no indications whatsoever of a relationship. An inspection of the row means for each level of SI, at each control level of AS, reveals no consistent patterning which can be taken as evidence for the existence of a relationship. We are forced to conclude, then, that there is no relationship between SI and alienation. In summary, and at least for the first order controls, we must reject our original hypotheses of significant differences between various levels of Status Inconsistency with respect to alienation. There appear to be few, if any differences. Control- ling for the effect of AS upon SI does not produce the expected relationship, and, therefore, we conclude that SI has no effect on alienation. Social Mobility Relationships Tables 57-62 detail the relationship of SM to alienation, while controlling for the effect of SI; and Tables 63—68 detail the relationship of SM t0 alienation while controlling for the effect of AS. An inspection of Tables 57, 59 and 60 reveals that——in 8 of 9 cases-it is the "Stable" group which has the highest level of alienation. This finding, of course, may be mapmpm emanates mo 44:64 seem Fm mmtzmmthqacn :4 5.11414 11111 4 1.4.1. it 1:15 168 44. 44F 44 4.4N 4 N.F4W 4F 4.44 NF N 444V 4F. ooF N4 F.4F 4F F.44 N4 N.o4 F4 F 4.FsmNmV 44. 44F 44F F.4F 4F o.o4V 44 4.44 44 o Aqu 4F. ooFW F4F 4.FF 4F 4.F4W 44 4.44 44 N FNNV 4F. ooF F4F o.NN F4 F.44 44 4.44 F4 F 4 4F. ooFV om 4.4F FF o.o4V 44 F.F4 F4 0 Aqu 4a. ooF 4FF N.FN o4 F.44 on o.F4 44 N FNNV Fm. ooF F4F m.oN 44 F.44 44 4.4N 44 F N 4F. 44F 44 F.FN 4 4.44 4F F.N4 4F c Fqu 44. 44F 44F o.4N m4 F.44 oF 4.FN F4 N AFmV 4o.F ooF 4NF 4.4N ‘44 N.n4 4m 4.4N N4 F F Fm. ooF N4 N.FN F 4.44 4F N.F4 4F 4 Fqu NF.F ooF N4 N.44 4N 4.44 44 4.FN 4F N ANmV 4F.F ooF 4F 4.44 44 4.4N 4F F.44 4N F o FaonV 44.F ooF F4F F.N4 44 4.44 44 4.4F NN o Fqu m as s as a ARV a 3 z ..aaaoeaa 8:84 :64 NaFoa N FNNFNV F o Apqu aSFeFm eseoem omdnobd 44424434448 4544.43 owonobd no debug soda so noose-oasokom .3. 424332303” 85st ...m canon. gvfipm OWUHOurdN MO HObOA Scam Fwd EC4+1r<1b .. 169 4F. 04F 44 F.4FV 4 F.F4 4F AN.44W 4F N 444V F4. ooF N4 4.FFW 4F 4.44 N4 MF.4N 4N F 4 ASNNNV 44. 44F 44F N.FN F4 4.44 4F 4.4NV F4 4 Fqu 44. ooF FoF 4.4F ON 4.44 F4 4.4N 4N N 444V 44. ooF N4F F.4N .F4 N.F4 F4 F.4N 44. F 4 44. 44F 04 4.FF 4F 4.F4 44 4.44 F4 4 Aqu F4. ooF 4FF 4.4F NN 4.44 N4 4.FN N4 N 444V 44. SF N4F F.4N 04 4.44 04 4.4N N4 F N 44. 44F 44 4.4F 4 4.F4 NN 4.4N 4F c Aqu F4. ooF F4F 4.4F 4N 4.44 44 4.FN 44 N 444V oo.F ooF 4NF 4.FN FN N.F4 NF 4.FN FN F F 4o.F ooF N4 N.F4 oF 4.44 4F 4.FN F o FoaV 44. ooF N4 4.4F 4F F.F4 44 F.FF 4F N 444V. 4o.F ooF 4F 4.4N ON 4.44 44 F.FF 4F F 4 Asqu 4F.F ooF N4F 4.44 44 4.44 4F 4.4F 4F c FoaV .4 S a S a 44 z 44 a ..524 42.43 364 Napoe N 44444V F 4 Faqu oapapm 494444 amenabd ZOHE< MO H0>®Q 2044444 n%.: uqqcaaflH—IE‘V\Lp {t'a‘ul“ lull ‘I4C ‘Iu (F‘ap‘E 171 F4.F 00F 44 M4.N F M4.4N 4 N.F4 4F 4.4N 4 4.4 N N 4444 N4.F ooF N4 4.4 4 4.4N NN 4.N4 44 N.FN 4N 4.NF FF F 4 444444 44.F 04F 44F 44.4 4 AF.4N o4 F.N4 F4 N.4N 44 4.F FF o 4044 44.F ooF FoF 4.4 oF 4.4F 4F 4.44 44 F.4N 44 4.4 v4 N A444 44.F ooF F4F F.4 4 N.4N F4 F.44 44 4.4F 4N 4.4 W4F F 4 FF.F 04F 04 4.4 4 0.0N 4F 4.44 F4 4.4N NN F.4F 4F 0 4°44 NN.N Moon4FF 4.4F 4F 4.4N 44 M4.F4 44 M4.4F FN 4.F 4 N n4mv 4N.N 00F 44F 4.4F 4N 4.4N 44 4.F4 04 F.4F 4N 4.4 4 F N 44.F 444F444 4.4 N 4.F4 NF 44.4N FF Ao.NN 4 N.4F 4 4 4°44 44.N ooFW44F 0.0N444 0.4NWN4 4.4N 44 40.4FW4N AF.4 oF N 4444 44.N ooF 4NF 4.4Fw4F 4.44 44 4.FN 44 A4.4F 4F m4.4 4 F F 04.F ooFVF4 4.NF 4 4.NFV4 F.44 NF 44.NNvF 4.NF 4 0 4°44 N4.N AoonN4 AF.44V4N 44.4NVFN 4F.F4 4N 44.4 4 AF.4 4 N 4444 0F.N Moon4F Mo.4NwFF A4.F4W4N Mo.FN 0N A4.oF 4 M4.F F F 0 43444 44.N ooF FNF 4.44 44 AF.44 N4 F.4F 4N Ao.FF 4F 4.N 4 o Aoqv m E 4 Q; 4 as 4 444 2 g z 444 z .5884 42448 404 44404 4 444444 4 N F 0 44044 424444 454444 mmdnm>< mmflmequMOZImmmzmmmnmmaom 4:444m mmwno>4 Mo Hm>mq noun 44 44mammoaaHOZImmmnmmoHnmspm.49 hocopmfimnoqu mspdpm .4m @4949 A‘v’erag 172 Table 55: Status Inconsistency by Present Standing at Each Level of Average Status Average Status Status Mean Control Inconsis. PRESENT STANDING (Lo) 0 6.44 (Hi) 2 6.73 (Lo) 0 6.03 1 6.54 (Hi) 2 6.54 (Lo) 0 6.71 1 6.80 (Hi) 2 6.90 (Lo) 0 7.18 1 7.16 (Hi) 2 7.22 (Lo) 0 7.21 (High) 4 1 6.85 (Hi) 2 7.44 Table 56: Status Inconsistency by Future Orientation at Each Level of Average Status FUTURE ORIENTATION Average Status Status Pessimistic Status Optimistic Total ng Control Incon. (O) Quoé1) (2) N (%) .X N (72) N /O N (%) (Lo)0 16(12.7; 51(4o.5) 59(46.8) 126€1oo) 1.34 (Low)0 1 6( 8.3 32( 4.5) 34(47.2) 72 100) 1.39 (Hi)2 9(11.5) 34(43.6) 35(44.9) 78(100) 1.33 (Lo)o 2( 6. 7) 13E43.3) 15(50.o) 30(100) 1.43 1 135 7.3) 53 43.1) 61(49.6) 123(100) 1.42 (Hi)2 9.9) 66(46.5) 62(43.6) 142(100) 1.34 (Lo)o 1( 2.6) 19(5o.o) 18(47.4; 38(100) 1.45 2 1 9( 5.7) 66(41.5) 84(52.8 159(100) 1.47 (Hi)2 8( 7.3) 52(47.3) 50(45.4) 110(100) 1.38 (Lo)0 5( 5.7) 21(3o.7) 56(63.6) 88(100) 1.58 3 1 8% 5.7) 60(42.9) 72(51.4) 140(100) 1.46 (31)2 6 6.0) 36(36.o) 58(58.0) 100(100) 1.52 (Lo)o ' 7< 5.0) 55(39.o) 79(56.o) 141(100) 1.51 (High)4 1 4( 4 4) 23(25.3) 64(7o.3) 91(100) 1.66 (Hi)2 2( 6.1) 13(39.4) 18(54.5) 33(100) 1.48 Fable Status Inconsi: Control “ M (Low)o lab Statu #31004 c 43.. 173 Table 57: Social mobility by waerlessness at Each Level of Status Inconsistency POWERLESSNESS Status Inconsis. Social (Low) 0 1 (High) 2 Total Raw Control Mobility N (fi) N (A) N (t) N (fi x Down 15E44.1; 17 50.0?8 $é 5 9) 34(100) .62 (Low)O Stable 95 33.7 100 35.5 0.8) 87 100; .97 Up 33(41.8) 32 40.5) 14(17. 7) 79(1oo .76 Down 27551.9) 16 30.8g9 917.3) 52(1oo) .65 1 Stable 106 29.0) 163 14.7 96 26.3) 365(1oo) .97 Up 49(39.2) 45 36.0) 31 24.8) 125(1oo) .86 Down 1o(28.6) 15(42.8) 1o(28 6 35(100) 1.00 (High)2 Stable 93€3o.5) 129 42.3) 85227 2 305 100) .97 Up 45 36.0) 55(44.0) 25 7o 0 125(100) .84 Table 58: Social Mobility by Social Isolation at Each Level of Status InconsistenCy SOCIAL ISOLATION Status Inconsis. Social (Low) 0 1 (High) 2 Total Now Control Mobility N (%) N (N) N (%) N (%) x Down 9(26.5) 20(58.8) 5(14.7) 34(100) .88 (Low)0 Stable 68(23.9) 144(50.7) 72(25.4) 284(100) 1.01 Up 22(27.8) 39(49.4) 18(22.8) 79(100) .95 Down 12(22.6) 29(54.8) 12(22.6) 53(1oo) 1.00 1 Stable 89(24.2) 195(53.o) 84(22.8) 368(100) .99 Up 32(25.6) 6o(48.0) 33(26.4) 125(100) 1.01 Down 7(2o.o) 24(68.6) 4(11.4) 35(1oo) .91 (High)2 Stable 69(22.6) 184(60.1) 53(17.3) 306(100) .95 Up 37(29.6) 61(42.8) 27(21.6) 125(100) .92 aunt? 1.1! \ I n 1 3 _ i .... 1 l x 1 i a a 4 n O a x 11 i v .- C 174 N4.F Aoon 4NF 44.444 N4 ”N.44V 44 AN.F4V 44 44 N4.F Aoon 444 4N.F4v 44F 44.F4v 4FF A4.4FV F4 o44444 N 444444 44.F 444F v 44 44.4NV 4F 44.44V FF A4.NNV 4 egon 44. Foon 4NF F4.4Nv 44 44.F4V N4 44.4Nv F4 44 FF.F Aoon F44 44.444 F4F 44.444 44F 44.FNV 44 444444 F NF.F Aoon N4 44.44V 4F 44.N4V NN F.4NV NF eaon F4. Aoon 4F AF.4NV 4N AN.44V F4 4F.F4v 4N as F4.F Foon F4 44.444 F4 4F.F4V 44F A4.FNV 4F o44e44 ,4 Aaoqv 44.F A44FV 44 44.4NV 4F AF.44V 4F F4.4NV 4 neon M 84 2 34v 2 A43 2 44V 2 44444434 494454 gem 444oe 444444 N F o Agoqv He4oo4 .444cooaH 444444 444444444442 hocopmamnoocH mdpdpm Mo Hoboq nomm 44 mmocmmoaanoz an 44444903 Hwfloom .mm @4949 175 44.N Aoon 4NF 44.NFv4F 44.4Nv44 4N.FNV44 44.NNV4N 44.4Fv4F A: 44.N 444Fv 444 44.4F444 44.4N44F AN.4FV44 A4.FFv44 44.4 VFF 444444 N4444mv 4N.N Aoon 44 44.4Fv4 44.NNV4 _ 44.4444F 4F.FF44 4F.4 VN asoa 44.N Aoon 4NF 44.4 vF 44.44444 44.4N4F4 44.4Nv4N 44.4Fv4F 4: 4N.N 444Fv 444 44.4FVF4 “F.FNVFoF 44.44VF4F 44.FF444 44.4 44F 444444 F 44.N 444FV F4 44.FFv4 44.FNVFF 44.44VFF 44.FNVFF 44.FFv4 .eaon 44.F Aoon 4F 44.4 44 44.4N44N “N.44VF4 “F.4NV4F 44.F v4 44 FF.N Aoon 4FN 44.4Fv44 44.NNV44 44.N4VF4 4F.4Fv44 44.4 v4N 444444 443o4v 44.F Aoon 44 44.4 4N 44.4Nv4 44.4Nv4 44.N4VFF -44.4 44 auon m 443 z 444 z 444 z 444 z 444 z A44 4 44444424 42448 Bo4 444oe 4 44444V 4 N F 4 Aso4v 444oo4 .4444o644 444444 mmfizmmflqfimOZImmEHmmagom hocopmfimcoonH mapepm mo Hoboq snow 44 444:44048402144mammoanokbm hp 444444os 444664 .44 44444 176 Table 61: Social mobility by Present Standing at Each Level of Status Inconsistency Status Kean Inconsistency Social Control Mobility PRESENT STANDING Down 7.09 (Low) 0 Stable 6.76 Up 7016 Down 6.51 1 Stable 6.72 Down 6094 (High) 2 Stable 6.84 Table 62: Social Mobility by Future Orientation at Each Level of Status Inconsistency FUTURE ORIENTATION Status Pessimistic Status Optimistic Total Row Incon. Social (0) Quoé1) (2) ._ Control Mobility N (fé) N 'o N . (‘73) N (%) X Down 1( 2.9) 19(55.9) 14(41.2) 34(100) 1.38 '(ng) Stable 23( 8.4) 106(39.o) 143(52.6) 272(100) 1.44 Up 3( 3.9) 26(33.3) 49(62.8) 78(100) 1.59. Down 2( 3.8) 27(50.9) 24(45.3) 53(100) 1.42 1 Stable 26( 7.3) 139(38.8) 193(53.9) 358(100) 1.47 Up 7( 5.7) 50(41.o) 65(53.3) 122(100) 1.48 Down 6(17.6) 16(47.1) 12(35.3) 34(100) 1.18 '(High) Stable 22( 7.5) 124(42.o) 149(50.5) 295(100) 1.43 2 Up 9( 7.6) 51(43.2) 58(49.2) 118(100) 1.42 177 44. M44Fw 4FF M4.4Fw 44 M4.44W 44 MF.44W F4 44 44. 44F 44F 4.4F 4F 4.44 44 4.44 44 444444 4 A4444v 44. 444FV FF 44.4 V F 44.444 4 44.44V 4 4444 44. 44Fv 44F 4.4F 44 4.44 44 4.44 44 44 44. 44FW 44F 4.44 44 4.44 44 F.44 44 444444 4 44. 44F 44 4.4F 4 4.44 «F 4.44 4F 4444 44. 44F 44 4.44 4F 4.44 44 4.44 4F 44 F4. 44F 44F F.44 44 4.44 F4 4.44 F4 444444 4 44. 44F 44 N.4F 4 4.44 4F 4.44 4F 4444 4F.F 44Fw 44 4.44 4F 4.44 4F 4.44 4 44 44. 44F 4FN 4.44 44 4.44 44F 4.44 44 444444 F 44. 44FV 44 4.44 4 14.44 FF 4.44 NF 4444 44.F 444FV FF 4.44w 4 44.444 4 44.44 4 44 44.F M44FW 444 4.44 44F M4.~4w 44 M4.Fm F4 444444 4 44444 44. 44F 4F F.4NV 4 F.44 4 4.44 4 4444 44 $3 4 4444 4 44$ 4 44$ 4 44444444 44.44444 444 44444 4 44444v F 4 4444v 444444 444444 onHo>4 4444444444444 mfivdvm omdnmhd no Ho>oq scam pd muoqmuoanokbm an 44444902 444444 .44 44444 178 44. 4FF 44.4F mm 4.44 44 4.44 44 44. 44F M4.4F 4N F.44 F4 F.44 444444 4 444444 44.F FF 4.44 4 4.44 4 4.44 4444 44.F 44F 4.4m 44 4.44 44 4.44 44 44. 44F 4.44 44 4.44 44 4.44 444444 4 F4. 44 4.4F 4 4.F4 F4 4.44 4:44 4F.F 44 4.44 F4 4.44 44 4.4F 44 F4. 44F 4.F4 44 4.44 F4 4.44 444444 N 44. 44 F.4F 4 4.44 4F 4.44 4:44 44. 44 .4F 4 4.44 4F 4.44 44 44. F44 .F4 44 «.44 «NF F.44 444444 F 44.F F4 .4F 4 4.44 F4 4.NF 4444 F4. FF M .4FW m 44.44 4 M4.44W 44 FF.F 44m .44 44 44.44 44F 4.4F 444444 4 44444 44.F 4F 4 .44V 4 44.44v 4 4F.44v 4444 M 4 4 443 4 44444444 4444444 444 44444 4 44444v F 444444 444444 03.425 444444444 444444 444444 4444444 44 44444 4444 44 444444444 444444 44 44444444 444444 .44 44444 179 on. Aoorv m". Am.om om v.mv mo ..om on no mm. Moopw pm? MN.FN mm ~.mo no F.Nm on annopm o Anmnmv 00.? no? FF m.nm m o.mv m m.- m nson mm. Aoopw mo. m.mmw om m.mm mm Ao.o¢w me no om. Aoo. fin? N.FN mo o.mm mo Am.mm on annoom m 00., AOOFV on o.mmv or N..v v. Ao.mmv o. coon Nr.. Moo, mm An.~m om .N.mm mm w.mm m? nn m... 00? mm? Mo.mm mo v..v n» o.m~ .o annopm m Fm. Aoo. mm N..N n m.¢m m? N.¢m m neon no.n oowv om An.om n. m.mmV or 0.0. n no mm.. ooww rum Mm.¢o mm m.mmw mp m.mn ow cannon . oF.P co. Fm ~.mm m. n.am m? o.~m n soon 00., Aoopv Fr Mv.om o Mm.om o Mm.- n no mm.. Moonv mmm ..mn pm. o.mm an m.» n. annunm o Agony mv.. oopv Nn Ao.om o . A~.Fo m Am.w F nnon m E z E z E z E a 3:303 H238 son nopon m flownmv P o Anonv nonoom msponm owdno>< mmnnmmnnnmoz mapdpm omwno>4 no Hmboq nomm pd mmocmmoaanoz an hpaflfipoa Hdnoom .mm oapde 180 mw.P Aoonvmpn An.m Vm Am.mwvom Am.~mvmn Am.mmvnm A>.m vnn no nw.n Aoovwomn An.m vm An.mnvom An.onvmm Ao.mmvwm Am.m vm. onnopm n Anmnmv mo.m Aoon Fr Ae.m VF Am.~mvm Am.nmvm Am.omvn Ao.o vo noon mn.. Aoonvoow Aw.m Vn Ao.mmvmm Ao.mmvmm Am.nmvmw Am.mnvmn . nn mm.r Aoonvonn Am.» vnn no.nmwwm Am.mmvon Am.nmwwm An.m Won onnnpm m mn.n Aconvnm Am.w vm An.nn m Am.wmvmw Am.mm w A~.nn m noon NN.N Aoonvmo A».m vo MF.mmVnm An.omwmn An.nnvmn An.w Wm nn NN.N Aoonvmmn Ao.nnvom m.nmvon Am.om mo Am.n.vmm Ac.» m. onnnpm m pm.n Aoonvmm An.m Vm Am.mrvm An.mnvnn Am.omVo. Ao.m V? nnon nm.m Aoon om m.mm on Ao.onvmn o.onvm Am.mnwn An.m W? n: ww.m Moon arm m.mr nm Mm.mwwrm n.mmwnn Mn.nn mm Am.m m, onnnpm n on.m no? on n.0, m n.mm m «.mm P n.onvm A>.onvm noon mm.m floor Pr Am.mnvm An.omvn Mn.mmvn Ao.o Vo Ar.m n n: mm.m Moon rmm Mm.mmwm> Mm.0mWon m.nmvmm Mm.onvmm An.r n onnopm o Agony no.m oor NF n.w F m.mm n Ao.nmvm m.m V? Ao.o o neon m E z E z E z E n E z E z 323% 3350 son nonon n Anmnmv m N F o Anonv nonoom onpnpm mmmno>< mmnzmmnnnnoznmmnnmmnnmnnon mapmpm ommnobd mo Ho>oq nomm pm mmozmmoaanozlmmonmmoanokpm hp hpwanpofi HdHOOw a mm OHPGB 181 Table 67: Social Hobility by Present Standing at Each Level of Average Status Average Status Social Mean Control Mobility PRESENT STANDING Down 5.77 (Low) 0 Stable 6.52 Up 6073 Down 6.61 1 Stable 6.5? Up 6.40 Down 6.63 2 Stable 6.67 Down 7.24 3 Stable 7.19 Down 7.64 (High) 4 Stable 7.13 Up 7.04 Table 68: Social Mobility by Future Orientation at Each Level of Average Status FUTURE ORIENTATION Average Social Pessimistic Status Optimistic Total Row Status Nobility (o) QuoE1) 2) __ Control N (96) N %) N (96) N (7.) 1: (Low) Down 7.7 8 61.5 4530.8 13 100; 1.23 0 Stable 24 10.7 90 4o. 2 110 49.1 224 100 1.38 Up 0 0.0 770.0 3(30. 0 10 100) 1.30 Down 5 16.1 238. 7) 14(45. 2 31(100) 1.29 1 Stable 19 8.9 645.1; 98(46. 0 213E100 1.37 Up 1 3.9 453. 8; 11(42. 3 26 100) 1.38 Down 2 6.2 959.4 11 34. 4 32 100 1.28 2 Stable 9 4.9 $42.9 96 52. 2 184 100 1.47 Up 6 9.2 $44. 6 3o 46. 2 65 100 1.37 Down 1 2.9) 55 9) 14 41. .2; 34(100) 1.38 3 Stable 12 7.0;? 934 3g1o1 58. 7 1725100 1.52 Up 6 5.7 138. 7) 59 55. 6) 106 100) 1.50 Down 0 0.0 436.4) 72 63. 6 11§1oo 1.64 (High) Stable 7 5.3 534.1 802 60. 6 132 100 1.55 4 Up 6 5.4 632.4 69(62. 2 111(1oo 1.57 182 due to the effect of AS. Tables 58, 61 and 62 reveal no consis- tent patterns. Our conclusion, then, is that SM may be related to alienation, but that this pattern may also be accounted for by AS. An inspection of Tables 63—68 reveals that no consistent relationship exists between SM and alienation when AS is control- led. When taken in context of the previous set of tables, it becomes obvious that AS accounts for the previous relationship of SM to alienation, i.e., because "Stable" persons are also the ones with the lowest AS. This pattern is retained, however, at the lowest level of status. (At the lowest level of status, those persons who are "Stable" are the most alienated, when, given the control for AS, we would not expect this.) It may be that those persons in this lowest AS stratum who are downwardly mobile remember what it was like (when they were kids), and those who are upwardly mobile think that they will get out, and, there- fore, do not become alienated. This is not, however, borne out in Table 68, where the low AS "Stable" persons have the highest ipercentage of people in the "Optimistic" category, whereas—— if our speculations are correct—awe should expect that the "Up" SM group would have the highest percentage. It may be, then, that this "exception" is only a chance exception. In summary, we must note that of all three independent vari— ables, it is clear that AS is the only one that is consistently related to alienation, and that if the other two variables (SM and SI) are related to alienation, it is due to the confounding 133 effect of AS. Before we can state definite conclusions, how- ever, we should examine the third order tables to see if the above relationships-—and the lack of relationships-—obtain. Third Order Control Relationships Introduction In this section, we shall examine the relationship of each of our three independent variables to our dependent variables, while controlling for the other two, e.g., we shall examine the AS relationships while controlling for SM and SI simultaneously, etc. Because each of these tables will produce 45 rows and, consequently, a huge number of cells (495 cells in the case of PS), we shall present only the row means, or the means of the dependent variable for each level (row) of the independent variable and its controls. In creating these tables, we might note that it is only necessary to create one "set” of tables for each independent variable. For example, using AS as the primary control and SM as the secondary control, is (with respect to the order of SI) equivalent to using SM as the primary control and AS as the secondary control. That is, shifting primary and secondary controls does not alter the arrangement of rows in the SI- dependent variable sub-table (see Figure 1). For these reasons, then, only one set of tables will be presented, for each independent variable. In terms of analysis, we shall inspect each sub-table, i.e., the table listing the relationship between the independent 184 Figure 1: Two Tables Illustrating Similarity of Various Control Variable Arrangements Dependent Variable Dependent Variable AS SN SI 0 1 2 SN AS SI 0 1 2 o 1 2 3‘\ o 1 2 3 D 1 4 5 6 0.. 1 4 5 6 2 7 8 91f '7 2 7 8 9 o 10 11 12 (o 28 29 30) O S 1 13 14 15 1 1 31 32 33 2161718 /2 343536 0 19 2o 21 0 U 1 22 23 24 D 2 1 2 25 26 27 2 o 28 29 30 o D 1 31 32 33 3 1 2 34 35 36 2 O 37 38 39 0 1 S 1 4o 41 42 4 1 2 43 44 45 2 0 46 47 48 91:00 U 1 49 50 51 2 52 53 54 etc. variable and the dependent variable at each level of the two con- trol variables taken together. Then, we shall attempt to make some statement referring to the presence or absence of a consis— tent pattern throughout all or a majority of the sub-tables. That is, if a table contains nine sub-tables (as does the AS-dependent variable, controlled for SI and SH), then a consistent pattern should ideally be present in all nine of the tables; we shall modify this, however, and expect only a "large majority" of con- sistent patterns throughout the sub-tables. Obviously, the analysis will also be subject to other types of judgments, e.g., 185 linearity, size of differences, etc. We shall endeavor to present these as explicitly as possible. Average Status Tables 69-74 list the relationship of Average Status to the dependent variables at each level of Status Inconsistency and Social Mobility. This control set produces nine sub-tables, each sub-table listing the relationship of AS to a dependent variable. An inspection of these sub—tables reveals that in the majority of cases (from five of nine to seven of nine sub- tables), there is a very general linear relationship between AS and alienation, both in terms of the sub-scales and in terms of PS and F0. This does not, however, hold true for Social Isolation, where in only one of the nine sub-tables does a linear relationship hold true. Otherwise, no consistent rela- tionship patterns exist between AS and SOI. The creation of a table with 45 rows, however, also raises the possibility that some of the rows will have a relatively small N.3 This is, in fact, true, and two rows have zero frequencies: "Low" SIé—"Up" SX—-"1" AS and "High" SI-—"Down" 51,. "High" AS. Others have frequencies as small as one, two, three or four (Table 69). A very small N produces a relatively high amount of variance, and an unstable estimate of the true popu- lation mean. When row means based upon an N of five or less were dropped, and then the linearity of the remaining means was inspected, it became apparent that a vast majority of sub- tables exhibited a general 1inearity—-in most cases around 186 Table 691 Average Status by Powerlessness Means with Controls for Status Inconsistency and Social Mobility Status Social Average POWERLESSNESS Inconsis. Mobility Status Total leans (low)o 4 1.00 1 2 .50 Down 2 4 .75 3 16 .56 (High)4 8 .50 (Iow)o 106 1.34 1 26 .85 (Low) Stable 2 25 .72 0 3 5o .82 (Hieh)4 1 75 .68 (Low)0 I 1 2.00 1 l o .00 UP 2 4 075 3 l 17 1.06 (High)4 57 .65 (lew)o ‘ 4 .75 1 15 .87 Down 2 17 .47 3 13 .54 (High)4 3 1.00 (Low)0 l 63 1.16 1 83 1.01 1 Stable 2 1 103 .98 3 1 73 .88 (Hish)4 1 43 .77 (an)0 3 .67 1 10 1.40 Up 2 25 1.00 3 47 .77 (High)4 i 40 075 (low)o 1 5 1.00 1 I 13 .85 Down 2 ' 12 1.08 3 5 1.20 (High)4 7 o .oo 1 110 .99 (High) Stable 2 4 59 .88 2 3 53 .83 (High)4 i 18 .83 (Low)0 7 1.00 1 20 1.00 3 42 ‘ .57 (Hieh)4 . 16 1 .94 187 Table 70: Average Status by Social Isolation Means with Controls for Status Inconsistency and Social Mobility SOCIAL ISOLATION Means Status Social Average Inconsis. Mobility Status 8‘ EL 0 .75 1.00 1.00 .75 1.13 Down (High) A 8‘ 3L 1.19 1.08 .88 078 .95 (Low) 0 Stable (High) 5‘ EL 1.00 .00 1.00 1.06 (High) .91 5‘ 3L 1.50 1.19 .71 1.08 .67 Down (High) (Inn) 1.10 .94 .98 .96 .98 1 Stable (High) 5‘ EL 1.00 1.00 1.20 1.06 .83 Up (High) .80 .92 .92 1.00 .00 (Low) Down (High) ,\ 5‘ EL 1.00 1.00 .80 1.02 .72 (High) 2 Stable (High) .86 .70 1.10 .90 .81 (Low) -b&MrO-hC)4>LHPO-*C>4>LflhD-‘c)4>LMnD-bc>4>LuhD-bC)4>LHDO-bc>4>b’hD-fic>4>LuhO-*C)4>Luro-b (High) 188 Table 711 Average Status by Normlessness Means with Controls for Status Inconsistency and Social Mobility Status Social Average NORMLESSNESS Inconsis. Mobility Status Means (Low) 0 1.75 1 .50 Down 2 .75 3 1.00 (High) 4 1.00 (Low) 0 1.49 1 .73 (low) 0 Stable 2 .88 3 078 4High) 4 .86 (Low) 0 2.00 1 .00 3 .65 . (High) 4 1.07 1 1.31 Down 2 .94 3 1.00 (High) 4 1.00 (LOW) 0 1049 1 1.25 1 Stable 2 1.17 3 .93 (High) 4 .93 1 1.60 3 096 (High) 4 .80 (Low) 0 1.00 1 1.08 Down 2 1008 3 1.00 (High) 4 .00 (Low) 0 1.58 1 1.37 (High) 2 Stable 2 1.22 3 1.11 (High) 4 .94 (Low) 0 .86 1 1.40 Up 2 1.18 3 .74 (High) 4 1.00 189 Table 72: Average Status by Powerlessness—Normlessness Means with Controls for Status Inconsistency and Social Mobility Status Social Average Pcwerlessness- Inconsis. Mobility Status Normlessness Means (Low) 0 2.50 1.00 2.00 1.75 2.00 2.86 1.81 1.84 1.66 1.79 (Low) 0 Stable 4.00 .00 1.50 2.00 1.91 Up 3.00 2.33 1.71 1.69 2.33 2.65 2.30 1 Stable 2037 1.91 2.67 3.30 2.16 1.96 1.73 Up 2.60 2.00 2.33 2.20 .00 Down 2.91 2.37 2.10 2.17 1.67 (High) 2 Stable 2.29 2.65 2.33 1.52 2.19 2 3 4 O 1 2 3 4 0 1 2 3 4 O 1 2 3 4 O 1 2 3 2.05 4 0 1 2 3 4 0 1 2 3 4 0 1 2 3 4 0 1 Up 2 3 4 190 Table 73: Average Status by Present Standing Means with Controls for Status Inconsistency and Social Mobility Status Social Average Present Standing Inconsis. Mobility Status Means (low) 0 6.25 1 6.50 Down 2 6.00 3 7.31 (High) 4 7.75 (Low) 0 6.45 1 6.08 (low) 0 Stable 2 6.56 3 7.18 (High) 4 7.23 (Low) 0 8.00 1 .00 Up 2 8.75 3 6.94 (High) 4 7.11 (Low) 0 3.75 1 6.44 Down 2 6.65 3 7.08 (High)_4 7.33 (low) 0 6.57 1 6.64 1 Stable 2 6.55 3 7.18 (High) 4 5-75 1 6.90 3 7.28 (High) 4 6.83 (low) 0 7.00 1 6.85 Down 2 6.82 3 7.40 (High) 4 .00 (Low) 0 6.59 1 6.63 (High) 2 Stable 2 6.93 3 7.20 (High) 4 7.56 (low) 0 6.57 1 6.15 3 7.14 (High) 4 7.31 191 Table 74: Average Status by Future Orientation Means With Controls for Status Inconsistency and Social Mobility Status Social Average FUTURE ORIENTATION Inconsis. Mobility Status Means (Low) 0 1.00 1.50 1.00 1.44 1.63 1.37 1.44 1.40 1.58 1.46 2.00 .00 1.50 1.59 1.59 1.50 1.44 1.47 1.23 1.67 1 2 3 4 0 1 2 3 4 0 1 2 3 4 0 1 2 3 4 0 1.38 1 1.37 2 1.51 3 1.44 4 0 1 2 3 4 0 1 2 3 4 0 1 2 3 4 0 1 2 3 4 Down (Low) 0 Stable Up 1 Stable 1.71 1.00 1.44 1.29 1.49 1.60 1.20 1.08 1.09 1.60 .00 1.40 1.36 1-44 1.56 1.56 1.29 1.35 1.41 1.48 1.40 Up (High) 2 Stable Up 1 1111'! 5 192 seven of nine possible tables, with SOI excepted, of course. An inspection of the various levels of control across the various major tables also reveals that-~with one exception- "error", or sub-tables in a level which did not fit our predicted linear pattern Were relatively randomly scattered and not concen- trated in any one level of control. The exception was for the "High" SIé-"Down" SM sub—table which produced a linear relation- ship only with respect to Present Standing, and even then, line- arity was based upon only two means, with the other three dropped for containing five or less cases. One might also note that there were only 34 persons at this level of control, so it may be a "deviant" case. We would conclude, then, that AS is related to alienation, at all levels of control for SI and SM. We would note, however, that there is a relatively large amount of variance in these linear patterns, and that no statistical significance testing was done. If such a statistical test should be devised, it might prove that these generally linear patterns were only spurious, and insignificant. Until such a test is developed, however, we would maintain that the relationship does, in fact, exist. Status Inconsistency Tables 75-80 depict the relationship of Status Inconsistency to the dependent variables while controlling for Average Status and Social Mobility, simultaneously. This control set produces 15 sub-tables, with each sub-table listing the relationship of SI 111 193 Table 75: Status Inconsistency by Powerlessness Means with Controls for Average Status and Social Mobility Average Social Status POWERLESSNESS Status Mobility Inconsistency Means (LOW) 0 1.00 Down 1 .75 (High) 2 1.00 (Low) 0 Stable 1 1.16 (High) 2 1.15 (low) 0 2.00 (High) 2 1.00 (LOW) O 050 Down 1 .87 (High) 2 .85 1 Stable 1 1.01 (High) 2 ~99 (km)0 .00 (High) 2 1.00 (Low) 0 .75 Down 1 .47 (High) 2 1.08 (km)0 .75 2 Stable 1 1.00 (High) 2 .68 (LOW) O .72 (High) 2 .98 (continued) 194 Table 75: (Continued) Average Social Status POWERLESSNESS Status Mobility Inconsistency Means (L9W) 0 .56 Down 1 .54 (High) 2 1.20 (LOW) 0 .82 3 Stable 1 .88 (High) 2 .83 (Low) 0 1.06 (High) 2 .57 (low) 0 .50 Down 1 1.00 (High) 2 .00 (Low) 0 .68- (High) 4 Stable 1 .77 (High) 2 .83 (LOW) 0 065 Up 1 .75 (High) 2 (L .94 195 Table 76: Status Inconsistency by Social Isolation Keane With Controls for Average Status and Social Mobility Average Social Status SOCIAL ISOLATION Status Mobility Inconsistency Means (low) 0 .75 Down 1 1.50 (High) 2 .80 (km)0 1A9 (Low) 0 Stable 1 1.10 (High) 2 *1.00 (LOW) 0 1.00 (High) 2 .86 (Low) 0 1.00 Down 1 1.19 (High) 2 ~92 . (low) 0 1.08 1 Stable 1 094 (High) 2 1.00 (low) 0 .00 (Low) 0 1.00 Down 1 .71 (High) 2 .92 (Low) 0 .88 2 Stable 1 098 (High) 2 .80 (10W) 0 1.00 (High) 2 1.10 (continued) 196 Table 768 (Continued) Average Social Status SOCIAL ISOLATION Status Mobility Inconsistency Means (law) 0 .75 Down ' 1 1008 (High) 2 1.00 (Low) 0 .78 3 Stable 1 .96 (High) 2 1.02 (low) 0 1.06 Up 1 1.06 (High) 2 .90 (km)0 1J3 Down 1 .67 (High) 2 .00 (low) 0 .95 (High) 4 Stable 1 .98 (High) 2 .72 (Low) 0 .91 Up 1 .83 (High) 2 .81 197 Table 77: Status Inconsistency by Normlessness Means with Controls for Average Status and Social Mobility Average Social Status NORMLESSNESS Status Mobility Inconsistency Means (lbw) o 1.75 Down 1 1.67 (High) 2 1.00 (Low) 0 1.49 (low) 0 Stable 1 1.49 (High) 2 1.58 (Low) 0 2.00 (High) 2 .86 (low) 0 .50 Down 1 1031 (High) 2 1.08 (low) 0 .73 1 Stable 1 1025 (IOW) 0 .00 (High) 2 1.40 (Rm)O {5 Down 1 .94 (High) 2 1.08 (low) 0 .88 2 Stable 1 1.17 ‘ (High) 2 1.22 (Low) 0 .75 Up 1 1.08 (High) 2 1.18 (continued) i 1...! 11. 198 Table 77: (Continued) Average Social Status NORMLESSNESS Status Mobility Inconsistency Means (Low) 0 1.00 Down 1 1.00 (High) 2 1.00 (Low) 0 .78 3 Stable 1 .93 (High) 2 1.11 (Low) 0 .65 Up 1 .96 (High) 2 .74 (low) 0 1.00 Down 1 1.00 (High) 2 .00 (Low) 0 .86 (High) 4 Stable 1 .93 (High) 2 .94 (Low) 0 1.07 (High) 2 1.00 La; 199 Table 78: Status Inconsistency by Powerlessness—Normlessness Means with Controls for Average Status and Social Mobility Average Social Status POWERLESSNESS— Status Mobility Inconsis. NORMLESSNESS Means (hm)0 250 Down 1 3000 (High) 2 2.60 (Low) 0 2.86 (Low) 0 Stable 1 2.65 (High) 2 2.91 (Low) 0 4.00 Up 1 2.67 (High) 2 2.29 (Low) 0 1.00 Down 1 2.33 (High) 2 2.00 (Low) 0 1.81 1 Stable 1 2.30 (High) 2 2.37 (low) 0 .00 UP 1 3.30 (High) 2 2.65 (low) 0 2.00 Down 1 1.71 (High) 2 2.33 (Low) 0 1.84 2 Stable 1 2.37 (High) 2 2.10 (hm)0 150 (High) 2 2033 (continued) Table 78: (Continued) Average Social Status POWERLESSNESS— Status Mobility Inconsis. NORMLESSNESS Means (Low) 0 1.75 Down 1 1.69 (High) 2 2.20 3 Stable 1 2.05 (High) 2 2.17 (Low) 0 2.00 Up 1 1096 (High) 2 1.52 (low) 0 2.00 Down 1 2.33 (High) 2 .00 (Low) 0 1-79 (High) 4 Stable 1 1.91 (High) 2 1.67 (low) 0 1.91 Up 1 1073 (High) 2 2.19 Table 79: Status Inconsistency by Present Standing Means with Controls for Average Status and Social Mobility 201 Average Social Status PRESENT STANDING Status Mobility Inconsistency Means (low) 0 6.25 (High) 2 7.00 (Low) 0 6.45 (Low) 0 Stable 1 6.57 (High) 2 6.59 (Low) 0 8.00 Up 1 5057 (High) 2 6.57 (low) 0 6.50 Down 1 6.44 (High) 2 6.85 (Low) 0 6.08 1 Stable 1 6.64 (High) 2 6.63 (Low) 0 .00 Up 1 5.90 (High) 2 6.15 (Low) 0 6.00 Down 1 6.65 (High) 2 6.82 (Low) 0 6.56 2 Stable 1 6055 (High) 2 6.93 (Low) 0 8.75 Up 1 7.52 (High) 2 6.68 (continued) 202 Table 79: (Continued) Average Social Status PRESENT STANDING (Status Mobility Inconsistency Means (LOW) 0 7.31 Down 1 7.08 (High) 2 7.40 (low) 0 7.18 3 Stable 1 7.18 (High) 2 7.20 (Low) 0 6094 Up 1 7.28 (High) 2 7.14 (LOW) O 7075 Down 1 7033 (High) 2 .00 (low) 0 7.23 (High) 4 Stable 1 6.76 (High) 2 7.56 (LOW) O 7011 Up 1 6.83 (High) 2 7.31 203 Table 80: Status Inconsistency by Future Orientation Means with Controls for Average Status and Social Mobility Average Social Status FUTURE ORIENTATION Status Mobility Inconsistency Means (Low) 0 1.00 Down 1 1.50 (High) 2 1.20 (LOW) O 1037 (Low) 0 Stable 1 1.38 (low) 0 2.00 (High) 2 1.29 (Low) 0 1.50 Down 1 1.44 (High) 2 1.08 1 Stable 1 1.37 (High) 2 1.35 (Low) 0 .00 (IOW) 0 1.00 Down 1 4.47 (High) 2 1.09 (low) 0 1.40 2 Stable 1 1.51 (low) 0 1.50 (High) 2 1.41 (continued) 204 Table 80: (Continued) Average Social Status FUTURE ORIENTATION Status Mobility Inconsistency Means (High) 2 1.60 3 Stable 1 1.44 (High) 2 1.56 (Rm)0 159 Up 1 1049 (Low) 0 1.63 Down 1 1.67 (High) 2 .00 (low) 0 1.46 (High) 4 Stable 1 1.71 (Low) 0 1.59 (High) 2 1.40 205 ‘MJEldependent variable. An inspection of these sub-tables reveals no consistent patterning in these relationships. With the possible exception of Table 77, no table contains more than three sub-tables (of 15 possible) which run in the predicted direction. In Table 77, which indicates the relationship of SI to Normlessness, there are six of fifteen sub;tableS'which run in the predicted direction, with the lowest level of SI also having the lowest alienation mean, etc. This linearity does not, however, carry over into Table 78, which depicts the rela- tionship of SI and the combined form of PN. Removal of means based upon five cases or less is not possible here, because this would leave one with only two means, which by definition would give one "linearity". Therefore, we are forced to conclude that there is no relationship between SI and alienation. Social Mobility Tables 81-86 characterize the relationship of Social Mobility to the dependent variables at all levels of Average Status and Status Inconsistency. This control set produces fifteen sub— tables, each indicating the relationship of SM to a dependent variable. An inspection of these sub-tables reveals no consis- tent patterns. In the original first order tables, there were indications that those persons in the "Stable" group had higher alienation; however, this relationship disappeared when AS was controlled for at the second order, and it appears to disappear in these third order tables also. Some indications Show that the "Down" group might be the least alienated; however, further 206 Table 818 Social Mobility by Powerlessness Means with Controls for Average Status and Status Inconsistency Average Status Social POWERLESSNESS Status Inconsistency Mobility Means Down 1.00 (Low) 0 Stable 1.34 Up 2.00 Down .75 (Low) 0 1 Stable 1.16 Up .67 Down 1.00 (High) 2 Stable 1.15 Down 005 (Low) 0 Stable .85 Up .00 Down .87 1 1 Stable 1.01 Down 085 (High) 2 Stable .99 Down 0 75 (Low) 0 Stable .72 Up .75 Down .47 2 1 Stable 098 Down 1.08 (High) 2 Stable .88 Up .98 (continued) Table 81: (Continued) Average Status Social POWERLESSNESS Status Inconsistency Mobility Means Down .56 (Low) 0 Stable .82 Up 1.06 Down .54 3 1 Stable .88 Up .77 Down 1.20 (High) 2 Stable .83 Up .57 Down .50 (Low) 0 Stable .68 Up .65 Down 1.00 (High) 4 1 Stable .77 Up .75 Down .00 (High) 2 Stable .83 Up .94 208 Table 821 Social Mobility by Social Isolation Means with Controls for Average Status and Status Inconsistency Average Status Social SOCIAL ISOLATION Status Inconsistency Mobility Means Down 075 (Low) 0 Stable 1.19 Up 1.00 Down 1.50 (Low) 1 Stable 1.10 0 Up 1.00 Down .80 (High) 2 Stable 1.00 Down 1.00 (Low) 0 Stable 1.08 Up .00 D0“ 1019 1 1 Stable 094 Down 092 (High) 2 Stable 1.00 Up .70 mwn 1000 (Low) 0 Stable .88 Up 1.00 Down .71 2 1 Stable .98 Up 1.20 Down 092 (High) 2 Stable .80 Up 1.10 (continued) 209 Table 82: (Continued) Average Status Social SOCIAL ISOLATION Status Inconsistency Mobility Means Down 075 (Low) 0 Stable .78 Up 1.06 Down 1.08 3 1 Stable 1.96 Down 1.00 (High) 2 Stable 1.02 Up .90 Down 1.13 (Low) 0 Stable .95 Up 091 Down 067 (High) 1 Stable .98 mwn .00 (High) 2 Stable .72 Up 081 210 Table 83: Social Mobility by Normlessness Means with Controls for Average Status and Status Inconsistency Average Status Social NORMLESSNESS Status Inconsistency Mobility Means Down 1075 (Low) 0 Stable 1.49 Up 2.00 Down 1067 (Low) 1 Stable 1.49 Down 1.00 (High) 2 Stable 1.58 Up .86 Down 050 (Low) 0 Stable .73 UP 000 Down 1031 1 1 Stable 1.25 Up 1.60 Down 1.08 (High) 2 Stable 1.37 Down 1075 (Low) 0 Stable .88 Up .75 Down .94 2 1 Stable 1017 Down 1.08 (High) 2 Stable 1.22 (continued) 211 Table 83: (Continued) Average Status 7 Social NORMLESSNESS Status Inconsistency Mobility Means Down 1 000 (Low) 0 Stable .78 Up .65 Down 1 .00 3 1 Stable 093 Up .96 Down 1.00 (High) 2 Stable 1.11 Up .74 Down 1.00 (Low) 0 Stable .86 Down 1 .00 (High) 4 1 Stable .93 Up 080 Down .00 (High) 2 Stable .94 Up 1.00 212 Table 84: Social Mobility by Powerlessness—NormlessneSS Means with Controls for Average Status and Status Inconsistency Average Status Social POWERLESSNESS- Status Inconsistency Mobility NORMLESSNESS Means Down 2.50 (Low) 0 Stable h 2.86 Up ( 4.00 Down 3.00 (Low) 0 1 Stable 2.65 Up 2.67 Down 2.60 (High) 2 Stable 2.91 Up 2.29 Down 1.00 . (Low) 0 Stable 1.81 Up .00 Down 2033 1 1 Stable 2030 Up 3.30 Down (I 2.00 (High) 2 Stable 2.37 Down 2.00 (Low) 0 Stable 1.84 Up 1.50 Down 1071 2 1 Stable 2037 Down 2033 (High) 2 Stable 2.10 Up 2033 (continued) 213 Table 848 (Continued) Average Status Social POWERLESSNESS- Status Inconsistency Mobility NORMLESSNESS Means Down 1.75 (low) 0 Stable 1.66 Down 1.69 3 1 Stable 2.05 Down 2.20 (High) 2 Stable 2.17 Down 2.00 (Low) 0 Stable 1.79 Down 2033 (High) 4 1 Stable 1091 Up 1073 Down .00 (High) 2 Stable 1.67 214 Table 85: Social Mobility by Present Standing Means with Controls for Average Status and Status Inconsistency Average Status Social PRESENT STANDING Status Inconsistency Mobility Means Down 6.25 (Low) 0 Stable 6.45 Down 3075 (Low) 0 1 Stable 6.57 Down 7.00 (High) 2 Stable 6.59 Up 6.57 Down 6.50 (Low) 0 Stable 6.08 Up .00 Down 6.44 1 1 Stable 6.64 Down 6.85 (High) 2 Stable 6.63 Up 6.15 Down 6.00 (Low) 0 Stable 6.56 UP 8.75 Down 6.65 2 1 Stable 6.55 Up 7.52 Down 6.82 (High) 2 Stable 6.93 Up 6.68 (continued) 215 Table 851 (Continued) Average Status Social PRESENT STANDING Status Inconsistency Mobility Means Down 7.31 (Low) 0 Stable 7.18 Up 6.94 Down 7.08 3 1 Stable 7.18 Up 7.28 Down 7.40 (High) 2 Stable 7.20 Up 7.14 Down 7-75 (Low) 0 Stable 7.23 Up 7.11 Down 7033 (High) 4 1 Stable 6.76 Up 6.83 Down .00 (High) 2 Stable 7.56 UP 7031 216 Table 86: Social Mobility by Future Orientation Means with Controls for Average Status and Status Inconsistency Average Status Social FUTURE ORIENTATION Status Inconsistency Mobility Means Down 1.00 (Low) 0 Stable 1.37 Up 2.00 Down 1.50 (low) 0 1 Stable 1.38 UP 1.00 Down 1.20 (High) 2 Stable 1.40 Up 1.29 Down 1050 (Low) 0 Stable 1.44 Up .00 Down 1.44 1 1 Stable 1037 Up 1044 Down 1 008 (High) 2 Stable 1.36 ‘ Up 1.35 Down 1.00 (Low) 0 Stable 1.40 Up 1050 Down 1047 2 1 Stable 1051 Up 1029 Down 1.09 (High) 2 Stable 1.44 Up 1041 (continued) 217 Table 86: (Continued) Average Status Social FUTURE ORIENTATION Status .Inconsistency Mobility Means Down 1.44 (Low) 0 Stable 1.58 Down 1.23 3 1 Stable 1.44 Down 1.60 (High) 2 Stable 1.56 Down 1.63 (Low) 0 Stable 1.46 Up 1.59 Down 1.67 (High) 4 1 Stable 1.71 Up 1.60 Down .00 (High) 2 Stable 1.56 [..--“I [1 p 218 inspection reveals an approximately equal number of tables where the "Down" mobile group is m2§t_alienated, as well as more or less random relationships. Again, as in the previous SI tables, it is impossible to remove those levels which contain five or less cases, because this would result in linearity by definition. We are forced to conclude, then, that there is no relationship between Social Mobility and alienation that cannot be accounted for by reference to AS. Conclusions Generally, this analysis found that the three independent variables of SI, AS and SM were inter-related, even when one variable is controlled for. This was taken as evidence for the necessity of control in any analysis attempting to utilize these three variables, and a validation of our previous attempts to demonstrate this need for control, logically. An examination of the relationships among our dependent variables-principally that of PS and F0 to P, N, PN and SOI—- indicated that both PS and F0 were related to our alienation sub-scales, but in different manners. First, SOI maintained a Significant but curvilinear relationship to PS and F0, which was taken as some evidence for the separability and independence of SOI from other sub-scales of alienation. Second, PS was directly related to alienation, whereas F0 maintained more of a curvilinear relationship. We interpreted this to mean that PS is part and parcel of our alienation phenomena, but that F0 is somewhat different. However, this interpretation could not 219 be fully tested in this analysis. An examination of our three independent variables, in terms of their relationship to the various indexes of alienation, indicated that AS was stably and consistently related to all but SOI. This was taken as evidence of (1) the separability of SOI, and (2) validation of our measures of alienation, i.e., that they are somewhat similar to those used by other persons, who also found alienation related to various measures of average status or social class. Finding that AS is directly related to F0 also casts doubt on our previous interpretation of the nature of the relationship between F0 and alienation. At this point, it would appear that FO-at least in terms of its rela- tionship to AS--is of a somewhat similar character as our other alienation sub-scales. Finally, SM and SI were not generally related to alienation. The only consistent finding which emerged from these tables was that the "Stable" SM group was in almost all cases the most alienated. However, it was reiterated that these 'lack' of relationships were to be expected, and not regarded as a test of hypotheses. The second order controls, however, must be regarded as partial tests of our hypotheses. These tables indicate that AS is related to alienation, but that AS and SI are not. The previously found relationships of SM to alienation is apparently accounted for by the effect of AS on SM. This general finding was supported by the third order tables, where AS was again related to alienation, whereas SM and SI 220 are not. With reference to our initial hypotheses, then, we are forced to reject them. We are left with the task of explaining the relationship of AS to alienation in some terms other than the traditionally used Durkheim—Merton rationale. This shall be the task of our final chapter. FOOTNOTES —— CHAPTER III 1. Unfortunately, this type of control was not planned as a part of the original analysis, and the extreme amount of advance planning necessary for computer analysis made it difficult to do after the analysis plans were built into the computer. 2. Dorothy L. Meier and Wendell Bell, "Anomia and Differential Access to the Achievement of Life Goals," American Sociologipal Review, 24 (April, 1959), 189-201. 3. These totals are given in Table 69; because they remain the same for all other tables in this sequence, they are not repeated. 221 CHAPTER iv CONCLUSIONS The basic problem of this thesis revolved around the Durkheim-Merton rationale underlying the theory of anomie, and its utility in explaining class differentials in anomie and other types of behavior labeled as pathological or adaptations to anomie. Basically, we argued that if this rationale did, in fact, account for the above-mentioned correlations-by reference to various structurally generated discrepancies-then it also should create correlations between alienation and other structurallyibased sources of discrepancy, i.e., status inconsistency and social mobility. Hypotheses to this effect were deve10ped. Ourtheoretical stance, then, gave us two alternatives, dependent upon whether these hypoth- eses were or were not rejected. First, if the hypotheses were not rejected, the theoretical interpretation would be that the Durkheimelertcn rationale was once more validated and extended to a new substantive area. Secondly, if the hypotheses were rejected, the theoretical alternative would then be to devise a scheme which would explain alienation, as well as the corre- lations of alienation and average status, in terms other than that of the discrepancy rationale. At the same time, this theory must also explain the correlation between average status and pathological behaviors, and previous findings of correlations 222 223 between status inconsistency, social mobility, and pathological behaviors. As was obvious in Chapter III, our hypotheses were not con- firmed; therefore, the second alternative must be taken. In so doing, we shall first consider previous critiques of aliens- tion and anomie theory, as they apply to any basic restructuring of anomie and social class theory. Then, we shall set about con- structing a somewhat different approach to this matter. In this different approach, we shall consider alienation, stratification, mobility and pathological behaviors. Critiques of Anomie Theory As stated previously in Chapter I, most critiques of anomie theory do not take to task the basic mechanisms by*which anomie (or anomia) develop-i.e., the discrepancy between that which is expected and that which is achieved. Rather, the major sugges- tions are that researchers lock for sources of discrepancy other than the very general discrepancy occurring between mass-culture goals, and societyuwide means of access to these goals. There is behind these criticisms a notion that each particular group, community, or organisation has its‘g=3_set of culturally pre- scribed goals (or better, gggroulturally specified goals), as 'well as its own stratification system which determines means of achieving those goals. A quote from James Short and his study of delinquency and gang boys in Chicago will perhaps exemplify this general type of critique; Detailed observation of particular behavior episodes suggests that . . . the conception of social structure re 224 which is basic to the [anomia] paradigm must be broad- ened to include situations which are more immediate to the boys, such as local community norms and Opportuni- ties, and normative and status considerations within the group, in addition to the abstract conceptions of Opportunity structures and status deprivations.1 Our comment on this type of critique of anomie theory would agree somewhat with that of Cohen, namely that it produces an atomistic and individualistic view of man, a view that is dis- tinctively non-sociological.2 It places the brunt of "develop- ing" anomie wholly "upon” the individual, in that he must first internalize certain social or cultural values (or sub-cultural or sub-sub-cultural values) which stand outside of him; go through a period of psychic conflict; develop anomie; and then act in an adaptive manner. It does not consider behaviors as a product of social relationships, but as individual preperties. Rather than elaborate in this vein, which is essentially a reduction to more and more microscopic levels, we would assume that this view has sufficient proponents. It will be our choice to deve10p (hopefully) a relatively new and different theory 'which approaches the problem via a concern with "societyawide” theory-—perhaps an oblique approach in current sociology. Suggested Theory Ib shall begin our explanation not with an understanding of anomie or alienation per se, but rather with an understanding of the nature of ”social class” and its correlates. This, we hope, will allow us to then explain the function, or consequences, of alienation within this theory. Further, we hope that it shall also be possible to examine mobility, etc. 225 We would argue that ”class" constitutes not an aspect of social structure, or a part of a unidimensional or monolithic structure, but essentially different structures having different cultures. These different class groupings must be articulated by reference to some sort of common mechanism. Class, then, is not a continuum, with more or less ordinal rankings along it, but a set of different nominal distinctions. That these various nominally distinguished groups happen to correlate with ordinal measures (e.g., education, income, etc.) is a matter to be explained, not to be taken as given, and therefore used as indexes of a unidimensional social class variable. Literally, we will argue that ”alienation” is nothing more or less than a part of a lower-class belief system. This approach begins with the work and conceptualisaticns of Herbert Cans, who argues that the lower class is an isolated social system which exists within, but not as a part of, the urban milieu.3 He argues that the lower class is characterized by a ”village” style of life, with the most important groups being the neighborhood and the peer group. That is, these are the major groups in the lower-class person's life, and they dom- inate his view of the world. The lower-class view of the world has as its mainstay, personalism-a personalistic orientation. By personalism, Cans refers to the styles of relating to other objects and people in terms of their appeal or meaning to the person rather than to any abstract characteristics of the object itself. By personalism, Cans also refers to seeing other persons 226 as more or less "whole" persons, rather than persons occupying a particular role. One interprets (and therefore understands) the actions of other persons by referring to the particular relationship that one has with the other. Cans goes on to discuss the way in which these lower-class persons participate in the various institutions which surround the lower-class person, i.e., school, jobs, buying practices, etc. Although the lower—class person participates in these institutions, Cans indicates that it is in a manner that is radically different from the middle class. He argues, for example, that the lower class view their jobs not as careers, or as a phenomena which is valued in and of itself, but more as a means of getting sufficient cash to come back into the neighbor- hood.4 In other words, those things that are important for the lower class are found in his neighborhood and his peer group. Lower-class man, then, with his extreme neighborhood and peer group orientation, his personalistic orientation, and his minimal (and somewhat forced) participation in the major urban institutions, exists in a Gemeinschaft village located in the Gesellschaft urban milieu. This, in turn, produces a form of isolation from the rest of society. In his treatment, Cans devotes little explicit attention to the way that this stratifi- cation system is maintained, or to the place of the middle class in the society. And, his discussions of the institutional life of the lower-class person are, to some degree, dominated by the lower-class person's view, to the exclusion of the middle-class functionaries view. 1' ("‘1 be 227 Sjoberg, Brymer and Farris have extended Gans' view of the lower class somewhat and have developed a conceptual schema which contains general mechanisms which create and maintain the stratification system and the isolation of the lower class.5 Generally, they argue that ”bureaucratic systems are the key medium through which the middle class maintains its advantaged position vis—a—vis the lower class."6 In effect, they are reversing the classic model of the relationship of bureaucracy and class. Rather than argue that social class creates differ- ential participation in bureaucratic organizations, they contend that bureaucratic organization, as a central feature in modern industrial society, creates differential participation in organi- zations, which in turn creates and maintains a stratification system. Implicit in their presentation is the assumption that the major portion of American life proceeds through bureaucra— tized organizations, e.g., employment, education, government, buying, etc. Staffing arrangements in bureaucratic organizations frequently place those least qualified persons in "hardship” or lower-class areas, so that loweraclass clients receive the least possible benefits of the organisation and, consequently, very few skills with which to escape the lower class. Bureaucracies, out of a necessity to maintain a successful image of themselves, vis-a-vis their supporters, frequently select those persons who are most likely to succeed rather than all persons who legitimately might participate in the organisation. That is, the organization must x2 228 exhibit a certain amount of goal attainment if it is to continue to succeed, so it selects as clients those persons who are most likely to succeed. And, these are very seldom lower-class persons (or, at least those persons who cannot succeed in the organization do not get the organisational benefits). Finally, the extreme specialization of any particular bureaucratic organisation-both as an organisation among organizations and within the organiza- tion-makes it somewhat difficult for organizational function- aries to view any particular client or production unit in a holistic fashion. Moreover, it prevents the bureaucratic func- tionary from taking the perspective of a client and from under- standing the client's view of his own problems. Literally, the specialized organisational functionary may not even be able to see any problem from the lower-class persons' perspective. we would argue that this contributes to a maintenance of lower classness. The lower-class person, who has little knowledge of the various informal and formal ways and byuways of bureaucratic functioning, is then called upon to negotiate with that highly specialised lower echelon person who is most rulebound—-and has little latitude in dealing‘with problems. And, if social problems are functionally inter-dependent, this means that the lower-class person must first negotiate a ”deal" with the most rulebound person, and secondly, coordinate the various specialized func— tionaries and organisations to fit his particular needs. The middle-class person, by virtue of his middle classness and his 229 knowledge of bureaucratic modes of Operation (derived from working in other bureaucracies) can go to a higher level functionary who has more latitude, and who can deal more effectively with any particular organizational problem that the middle-class person may have. The lower-class notions of personalism run head-on into the bureaucratic norms of impersonality and universalism. There- fore, when a lower-class person "gets something” from an organi- zation, he is likely to assume that it is because he had a per- sonal relationship with the functionary; if he does not, rather than ascribing it to failure to meet an impersonal formal rule, he is likely to ascribe it to "fate", ”luck”, or "chance”. This inability of the bureaucratised organisation to cope with the lower-class person-—and vice versa—-creates a vicious cycle of ”circular causation”, to use Hyrdahl’s term, which operates so as to create a relatively large amount of social dis- tancs between the middle and lower classes-or more aptly, the lower class and the rest of society.7 In other words, the ability of operating in a bureaucratic arganisation (or conversely, the ability of an organisation to respond to all types of persons) creates and maintains at least one class system which is isolated from the rest of the ongoing society. lore characteristics and elements of this theory will become apparent later. it first glance, the tenative theory under consideration would seem to be similar to those of Warner and Hollingshead.when they refer to class as representing a ”style of life”, and to Oscar Lewis' conceptualisation of a ”culture of poverty”.8 Closer I. L-\ 230 inspection, however, reveals some crucial differences. The biggest difference does not lie in the cultural content or belief system described by these authors, for they would agree with our observations. Rather, the difference would seem to lie in the mechanisms whereby such a system is maintained or the logical order to such a system when counterposed with the other "cause or effect“ variables. Hollingshead, for example, would appear to view social class as primary, and differential participation in major institutions as a resultant of social class. This is nowhere more clear than in his work, Social Class and.lental Illness, where chapter after chapter is devoted to ”the differential use of treatment facili- ties."9 In terms of a dynamic, or a mechanism whereby class is created and.maintained, Hollingshead would appear to rely upon a relatively vague notion of ”cultural transmission“, or ”like father, like son.” Lewis, while devoting much time to an. insightful description of the cultural habits of poverty strickp ened people, likewise pays little attention to the dynamics. creating and maintaining this culture. Generally, it would appear that he would subscribe to a definition of culture which views it as a form,of adaptation to an environment, in this case a hostile environment.10 Our suggested theory resembles Seaman's theory, in our mutual reliance upon bureaucracy as an important factor in developing ”alienation”.1‘ Seeman, however, does not discuss the role of social class as an intervening variable standing between bureauc- racy and alienation, or for that matter, social class as a 231 variable. Presumably, because of his "mass society” leaning, he would assume that'sll persons participating in modern life are subject to the whims of bureaucracy, and therefore to alienation. we might note, however, that his research has been carried out on, primarily, lower— and.working-class subjects, if we can judge from the occupations of his subjects, e.g., those in a reformatory, working in the lower echelons of a state hospital, or in labor unions. In summary, we are suggesting that stratification is created not by having education, money or a high prestige job, but by differential ability to participate in bureaucratic systems, as well as actual differential participation in these organisations. This differential participation creates-—at least for the present analysis-two major groups: those who can and do, and those who cannot and do not. The former are middle class, and the latter are lower class. Stratification, then, is a matter of a relationship——or the lack of a relationship—-betwesn a person and an organisation entity, rather than an individual attribute. Because‘gl; persons must come into some kind of contact with bureaucratised organisations in his life span, bureaucracy can then operate to maintain stratification systems. Lastly, we might note that considering bureaucracy as the primary determinant of stratifi- cation places this theory firmly in a larger theoretical context (namely, the theories regarding societal evolution from folk to urban to urban-industrial as a consequence of technological innovations.) Presumably, bureaucracy is one of the primary I‘ 232 innovations which allowed industrialism to develop.12 With this general and partial theory, let us re—examine the variables of alienation-anomie, the Durkheim—Merton rationale, status incon- sistency and social mobility, as well as the pathological behaviors attributed to social mobility, status inconsistency and lower classness. Alienation and Anomie In terms of the simple correlations of various types of attitude scales purportedly measuring "alienation” or "anomia”, 'we would argue that these are manifestations of aspects of the lower—class belief system. Rather than measuring "despair“, . as suggested by Heier and Bell, and Nettler, we would argue that they represent the consequences of a confrontation between a personalistic point of view, and impersonalistic bureaucra- cies."3 It would seem more appropriate, then, to label this phenomena ”fatalism" rather than despair, in that a fatalistic imputation is the mode of handling such incomprehensible events in a lower—class personalistic system.14 One might suggest, at this point, that if the statements making up our (and other's) alienation scales had had as referents events revolving around a lower-class neighborhood way of life, there might have been much less alienation in the lower class. In fact, it may be that in this case the middle class would have been more alien- ated. we might also comment on the lack of a relationship between the Social Isolation scale scores and Average Status. These I‘D 233 questions were phrased in terms of personal friendships, and feeling ”all alone in the world.” Given the lower-class orien- tation to neighborhood and peer group, it is hard to see how a lower-class person could‘ggg£_be without personal friendship ties. As Gone points out, the lower-class person has his primary existence in these peer groups. In summary, alienation or anomia, as indicated by various scales measuring powerlessness or normleesness, may be thought of as characteristic of a lowerbclass belief system. This does not, however, rule out the possibility of a more restricted conceptualization of alienation, particularly as used by Nettler and Hajda.1S This conceptualisation would deal primarily with the rejection of “culturally accepted goals”, and is, to some degree, synonymous with Merton‘s designation of "retreatism" as a form of adaptation to anomia. With respect to the discrepancy rationals underlying anomia, we might note that such a rationale is predicated upon the almost total acceptance of the culturally specified goals by a group, and, to the degree that we have argued for the presence of separate class and, thereby, cultural systems, it would be logically difficult for this complete consensus to occur. To this point, we are arguingHwith Hyman, when he attempted to demonstrate that various classes did‘ggg have a consensus‘with respect to goals.16 Of course, Horton's class reply argued that only a significgnt number must have internalized these culturally specified goals in order for a significant 17 amount of lower-class anomia, etc. /‘ "“'.o. "I 234 We would suggest a different resolution to this problem, and ask how it is that a lower class can have a multitude of goals, i.e., both "middle-” and "lower-“class goals, and.what effect this has. In our theory, we noted that the middle class is (as a group) in more or less control of the various bureaucracies with which a lower—class person must at some point in his life deal-showever effectively. it the same time, the lower-class person locks to the neighborhood and peer group for his values and his life satisfaction. It would appear, then, that the lower-class person must interact with ‘bglh_lcwer- and middle-class persons, and to some degree have adequate knowledge about the values of both. The middle-class person, however (because of his advantaged position, which allows him to dictate the terms of his various negotiations with the lower class), does not necessarily have to take into account the values of the lower-class person. That is, the middle-class person does not have to take into account the lower-class person's values in order to pursue a successful career. In fact, it would appear more likely that he must attend to the values of the organisation for which he works, which is very likely to be middle class.18 If this is true, than, we should expect that when confronted with a range of values, the lower class will espouse more than will the middle class. This is to some degree supported by Short in a study compar- ing the responses of middle- and lower-class adolescents to 235 various "middle-" and "lower-“class objects using a semantic differential technique.19 Objects which might be classified as "lower” or deviant-e.g., 'dcpe addict', 'pimp', etc.- were almost uniformly negatively evaluated by the middle class, and were neutrally or positively evaluated by the lower class. "Middle-class” objects-e.g., 'stable job', 'teachers', etc.- were equally positively evaluated by both middle- and lower— class adolescents. How, then, can this seeming contradiction exist? One possibility is suggested by Hyman Rcdman, who has developed the conceptualization of the ”lower-class value stretch”.20 Basically, Rodman argues that agreement with a wide range of values among the lower class is both functional for the lower-class’s environment (i.e., the middle-class bureaucracies), and accompanied by a lower degree of commitment to either sets of values. Literally, Badman is suggesting that the apparent discrepancy or contradiction is a product of a middle-class point of view, and that for the lowerbclass person, such a contradiction does not exist; it is a fact of life. In effect, than, we are suggesting that what may be a dis- crepancy may exist only in the perspective of the investigator and, therefore, cannot be automatically assumed. If a discrep- ancy were in fact perceived.by an individual, an anemic situation might result. However, this will have to wait further investi— gation. (C H...— ’6 236 Social Mobility,_iverage Status and Status Inconsistency Iith respect to the three structural variables, we must first note that our suggested theoretical system places primary emphasis upon one's relationship with bureaucratic systems.) Traditional indexes of social class or average status (and the derived concepts of status inconsistency and social mobil- ity) must then be interpreted'within this framework. For example, an occupation, per se, is usually carried out within a more or less bureaucratised framework, so that in order to hold a job, or advance to ”higher" jobs, one must have some degree of skill at operating within a bureaucratic framework. And, those jobs which require more skills (bureaucratic or otherwise) are usually attributed higher prestige, pay, etc. One must also attend.to the 'education‘ variable, which, in this framework, would seem to have a dual effect. First, one must frequently have a certain level of education in order to hold a position in a bursaucaatic organisation. Second, those persons who work in purportedly non-bureaucratic settings are usually persons who are “professionals“ (doctors, laeyers, etc.) although even this is changing more and.more rapidly. And, in order to obtain professional legitimacy, one must pursue a relatively long and arduous career through the educational system, which in turn requires a certain amount of bureaucratic skills in and of itself. So, even for the professional, a certain amount of bureaucratic skills is necessary, so that bureaucratic ability may be a highly important factor which I" 237 leads to other "indexes" of social class, e.g., money, etc. Social mobility also takes on a different cast when viewed in this perspective, particularly in terms of the ”causes" of mobility. Although Lipset, in his compendium on mobility in industrial society, recognized that “educational attainment is a major determinant of career patterns”,21 he does little to clarify or explain the particular mechanisms by which education operates so as to produce mobility, eg., "children from lowwstatus families do not have as much chance to stay in school as those from high-status families . . . If an individual comes from a working-class family, he will typically receive little education or vocational advice."22 That is, Lipset assumes that education is a key to mobility, but seems to assume that it operates to create mobility in such simplis- tic terms as vocational guidance, dropping out for economic reasons, etc. Further, ”poverty, lack of education, absence of personal 'ccntacts’, lack of planning and failure to explore fully the available job Opportunities that characterise the working-class family are handed down from generation to genera- 23 we would also note that all of these reasons are tion.“ somewhat individualistic in that they specify a failure of the individual as a reason for lack of mobility. Education, in our theoretical schema, would imply not only learning of substantive studies, but also learning a series of bureaucratic skills, which are transferable to other bureauc- racies, and which would presumably, increase the success one 238 might enjoy with those other bureaucracies. Because the educational system is one of the most heavily bureaucratised in modern society, and because every individual must at some time or other come into contact with it, the educational system is thus‘gpgn_to all, yet the most crucial and difficult of all to get through. A myriad of mechanisms operates to differentiate between lowert and middle-class persons as they move through this educational bureaucracy.24 First, we might note that the educational system is definitely a middle-class institution, if for no other reason than its staff. The lowest echelon person in an educational system is typically the classroom teacher, and she usually is required to have at least a college degree, and her occupational prestige is ranked in the middle to the upper-middle class. This in itself would make it difficult for the teacher to take the role of the lower-class pupil. Further complicating the matter is the notion that lower-class areas are frequently known as 'hard- ship' areas, and the worst or least experienced teachers are assigned to these areas (or are unable to get out), so that the lower-class child is confronted not only'with someone who is middle class, but who is also the least qualified to handle his case. And, because the lower-class parent has had little experience withlggy‘bureaucracies, he is unable to negotiate with the school system on behalf of his child.25 One might also note, however, certain factors in the educa- 239 tional system which serve to reinforce mobility patterns.26 Once a child is implicated in the educational system, and experiences some degree of success in it, he is drawn away from the neighborhood and the peer group and the ties which tend to hold him in the lower class are loosened. iOperating here, too, is the attitude of an out-of-schcol peer group to one of their members who is still in school. Generally, the student is the subject of some derision, and is given the label of a “square“; after a sufficient amount of time, the peer group begins to reject him, which leaves the student nowhere to go but back to the school for his social ties and reference group. Literally, the one who is mobile may be the deviant in the lower class. This general interpretation is supported by Gans in his discussion of mobility.27 He notes that those persons who leave the tightly knit neighborhood peer-group system are often viewed as 'traitors‘ or 'betrayers' of the correct way of life. A further distinction is added by his differentiation between group mobility and individual mobility. Group mobility usually has structural sources, e.g., the entrance of a new ethnic group on the scene, which moves the previous lower- class ethnic group up one occupational notch, or a period of rapid economic expansion, which allows everyone in this group to move up at once. In these 'group mobility. situations, there is little definition of each other as heretics. This would only occur with respect to individual mobility. 240 With respect to status inconsistency, this theoretical framework has little to offer. We note, however, that status inconsistency is predicated upon a definition of status in terms of multiple ordinally ranked dimensions. And, we have suggested that class is not a ranked order, but a nominal order, so that status inconsistency may be a spurious concept in a nominal system. Somewhat more positive, however, might be the suggestion to inspect various dimensions of status in terms of the degree to which they are either important or not important in structuring a person's relationship to bureauc- racies. That is, we might inspect various combinations of status dimensions to see if they might not form particular “types” with a particular organisation, or kind of organisation. Low ethnicity, for example, might create a peculiar kind of relationship to an organisation which might not be created by low education, etc. Pathological Behaviors The pathological behaviors which are associated with class, and to some degree with social mobility and status inconsistency and which are interpreted as stemming from anomie, must also be reinterpreted‘within the light of our suggested theory. ‘le would argue-as previously stated-that behaviors, pathological or not, are products of peculiar kinds of relationships between persons. Following Becker, we would note that someone, or some organisation, must first define deviance, in order for it to exist.28 The question would be to ask what kinds of relation- 241 ships between lower-class persons and bureaucratic organiza- tions obtain so as to produce more definitions for deviance for the lower class than for the middle class. Our theoretical framework would suggest two possibilities: first, that there is little actual difference in rates of commission of deviant acts between lower- and middle-class persons, but that the rate of definition by persons in positions to define is much higher for the lower class than for the middle class; and, second, following the notions of cultural differentiation, it may be that the lower class views as normal certain activities which are viewed by the middle class as deviant; this then produces conditions which lead to a higher rate of deviance or pathological behavior. That there may be little or no class differential in commission of deviant behaviors is partially supported—-at least with respect to Juvenile delinquency-by a survey of delinquency studies using selfereports as the measure of delinquency. In this survey Hardt and Bodine state that: lany studies have failed to demonstrate any associa- tion between the socie-economic status of Juveniles and.the incidence of delinquent behavior. That is, using the Juvenile's own report of his delin- quent behavior, rather than official records or statistics, leads to the finding that there are little or no differences between the classes with respect to delinquent behavior. However, when official statistics are used, a large difference is evident. h 242 How this comes about has been suggested by Piliavin and Briar, in regard to delinquency, who note that a Juvenile is much more likely to be picked up, as well as carried further into an ”official career” by police officers, if the person exhibits those characteristics defined by police officers as 30 "deviant". These characteristics include many characterisn tics which could also be defined as lower class, e.g., sloppy dress, bad language, etc. Other types of studies, for other areas of ”pathology" are also available.31 There is also the possibility that (given a large degree of cultural separation and isolation) what one class values, another class will consider deviant. And, if a group of people tend to define an activity as ”normal", it is likely that they will carry it out in more or less dpublic” situations, which increases the likelihood that it will be observed by those persons who are charged with making definitions of deviance based upon middle-class standards, i.e., the police, educators, etc. Walter Miller has suggested this general line of argument of his article, "Lower Class Culture as a Generating Milieu of Gang Delinquency".32 Others supporting this view are Rodman, Becker, Finestone and Lindesmith.33 With respect to the tenative associations between social mobility and pathological behavior, we can only put forth a suggestion. First, we would note that these associations have been sometimes contradictory, and that, in this thesis, no association was found between social mobility and alienation, so perhaps there exists no reason for such an adaptation to 243 exist.34 It may be, however, that distinguishing between group and individual mobility might be of aid. Gans suggests that those persons who are mobile as a group are not subject to any strain stemming from leaving the peer group, for everyone in the peer group advances at the same time as new ethnic groups enter the labor force, or as new areas of the economic system open up.35 Presumably, the same mechanisms could operate with respect to group downward mobility. ‘With respect to individual mobility, however, and especially upward indivi- dual mobility, the individual must literally leave the neighbor— hood and the peer group. And, he must suffer the rejection tendered him by his previous neighbors and peers. This may, in turn, produce a form of strain, which (while not neces- sarily a discrepancy) might lead to some form of adaptation or pathological behavior, especially mental illness or 36 neurosis. Conclusions and Proposals for Future Research ls have tested a set of hypotheses couched in relatively traditional sociological theory and have found them to be rejected. Then, we have suggested two alternative paths: first, further forays and elaborations of the traditional theory itself, and secondly, development of a new theory. By personal inclination, we would suggest that the second alternative places sociological theory much more firmly in the larger context of how societies develop and are held together. _-..-- 244 As for research, it would seem that there are several areas which take precedence at once. We have suggested that the middle class utilises a bureaucracy in order to maintain its advantaged position: yet, there is very little direct evidence to support this contention. This, in turn, immedi- ately raised the question of the place of an ”upper” class in this schema, and its mechanisms of maintenance. Also, the majority of examples in this paper are taken from the writer's own experiences and research in the field of delinquency. This would seem to point up the effect of bureaucracies in other areas of deviance, as well as the life cycle. For example, how does the adult come to be defined as mentally ill? Or as a criminal? Lastly, there would seem to be an area of exploration in terms of the operation of the bureaucracy itself, and perhaps areas of modern life which are not yet (or are only minimally) bureaucratised. Accompanying such a study might be a compari- son of client oriented bureaucracies with non-client oriented organisations. In any case, it is clear that much more empir- ical work must be done before this suggested grandiose theory can bear the explanatory fruit which will contribute to our understanding of the nature of social order. FOOTNOTES -’CHAPTER IV 1. James F. Short, Jr., "Gang Delinquency and Anomie,” Anomie and Deviant Behavior, Marshall B. Clinard (ed.), (Glencoe: The Free Press, 1964), p. 116. 2. Albert K. Cohen, "Toward a Theory of Deviant Behavior: Continuities Continued,“ Paper read before the American Sociological Association Annual Hosting, Los Angeles, California, August, 1963. 3. Herbert Gans, The urban Villagers (New York: The Free Press, 1965). 4. In this regard, Gans occupies a similar position to that of Robert Dubin, "Industrial Worker's Ibrlds,” Social Problems, 3 (January, 1956). 5. Gideon Sjobert, Richard A. Brymer and Buford Farris, "Bureaucracy and the Lower Glass,” Sociolc and Social Research, 50 (April, 1966), 325-337. 6. Ibide’ Fe 325' 7. Gunnar lyrdal, Economic Theo and Uhder-Develo d Regions (London: Gerald Ducksorth and Co., 1957), pp.16-20. 8.‘l. Lloyd‘Warner, it al., Democracy in Jonesville (New York: Harper, 1949). as well as other of his writings; August R. Hollingshead and Frederick C. Redlich, Social Class and Mental Illness (New York: John Wiley and Sons, 1953): and Oscar Lewis, Th: thldrgn 9f Sanchez (New York: Random House, 1961): Five Families (New York: Basic Books, 1959). 9. Here we are using Hollingshead only as an example of a traditional approach. Many others could also have been used. 10. Oscar Lewis, _o_p_. cit. 11. Melvin Seeman and John I} lvans, "Alienation and Learning in a Hospital Setting," Americgn Sociological Revigw, 27 (December, 1962), 772-782: lelvin Seeman, "Alienation and Social Learning in a Reformatory," American Journal 0 Sociology, 69 (lovember, 1963). 270-284; and Arthur G. Real and Melvin Seeman, ”Organisations and Powerlessness: A Test of the Mediation Hypothesis,“ American Sociolc lo 1 Review, 29 (April, 1964). 216-226. 12. H. R. Gerth and C. Iright Mills (eds. and trans.), From lax Webgr (New York: Olford University Press, 1946): Gideon Sjoberg, The Pre-Industrigl Cit; (Glencoe: The Free Press,1960). 245 246 13. Dorothy L. Meier and Wendell Bell, ”Anomia and Differential Access to the Achievement of Life Goals," American Sociological Review, 24 (April, 1959): 189-202: and Gwynn Nettler, "A Measure of Alienation," American Sociological Review, 22 (December, 1957). 670-677. 14. Other sources documenting this more or less fatalistic orien- tation may be found in Frank Riessman, Jerome Cohen and Arthur Pearl (eds.;, Mental Health of the Poor (New York: The Free Press, 1964 15. Nettler,‘gp. cit., and Jan Hajda, ”Alienation and Into- gration of Student Intellectuals,” American Sociolc ical Review, 26 (October, 1961), 758-777. 16. Herbert H. Hyman, ”The Value Systems of Different Classes: A Social Psychological Contribution to the Analysis of Strati- fication,” Class Status and Power, (eds.) Reinhard Bendix and Seymour H. Lipset (Glencoe: The Free Press, 1953), pp. 426-442. 17. Robert I. Horton, Social Theo and Social Structure, revised edition (Glencoe: The Free Press, 1957). 18. Sjoberg, Brymer and Farris,.gp. cit. 19. Robert A. Gordon, James 1". Short, Jr., Desmond S. Cartwright, and Fred L. Strodtbeck, "values and Gang Delinquency: A Study of Street Corner Groups,” American Journal of Sociolc , 69 (SCPtubCr’ .1963), 109'128e 20. Hyman Rodman, “The Lower—Class Value Stretch,“ Social Forces, 42 (December, 1963), 205-215. . 21. Seymour I. Lipset and Reinhard Bendix, Social Nobility in Igdustrial Society (Berkeley and Los Angeles: University of California Press, 1960), p. 197. 22. Ibid. 23. Ibide, Pe 198s 24. Only a few of these mechanisms can be mentioned here. Others, as well as an analysis of other institutional operations with respect to the lower class, will be presented in a manuscript in preparation by Richard A. Brymer and Gideon Sjoberg, "Bureaucracy, Community and the Lower Class”. 25. Sjoberg, Brymer and Farris, c ..gig. Also of interest here are the reasons for which a child leaves school. Most often, economic reasons are given. Yet, a survey of the "dropouts" themselves indicates that first, many of them have.positive feelings about the school, and second, few of them get jobs after dropping out. Their reasons for leaving school were that the school 'bugged’ them, etc. These findings might support the contention that the term ”dropout” has been coined by educators, and is used to place the responsibility for leaving.- school upon the child, and thereby remove any blame from the ..-. e , e I t ...... F e ' 2 t I C " 7‘ 247 from the school, which also would allow the school to retain its ”successful“ image. See various articles in Arthur B. Shostak and William Gomberg, New Pers ctives on Povert (Englewocd Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall, 1965). 26. Materials presented in this paragraph were derived from the files of the Wesley Community Youth Project, Wesley Community Centers, San Antonio, Texas, where the author is currently research sociologist. 27s OMB, m age, Chapter 8. 28. Howard 8. Becker, The Outsiders (New York: The Free Press, 1963). 29. Robert H. Hardt and George I. Bodine, Develo ent of Self-Re rt Instruments in Delin uenc Research (Youth Development Center, Syracuse University, 1965), p. 13. 30. Irving Piliavin and Scott Briar, "Police Encounters with Juveniles," American Journal 2f Sociology, 70 (September, 1964), 206-214. 31. Thomas J. Scheff, Bein lentall Ill: A Sociolc ical Theor (Chicago: Aldine Publishing Co., 1966): Hollingshead and Redlich, 33. $1.: Riessman, Cohen and Pearl, 33. 9_1_t_.: Shostak and Gomberg, 21. .933. . . 32. Whlter Killer, “Lower Class Culture as a Generating Milieu of Gang Delinquency,” Jgurnal 2f Social Issues, 14(1958). 33. Rodman, 32. 22.208 Becker, o . 933.; Harold Finestone, "Cats, Hicks and Color," Social Problems, 5 (July, 1957); Alfred B. Lindesmith and John Gagnon, ”Anomie and Drug Addiction," in Clinard, £2. $0, PP. 158.188e 34. Lipset, o .‘g£§., p. 71, notes that "while the notion that the socially mobile are more likely to be prejudiced against ethnic groups than the stationary has become rather common, the available evidence is quite ambiguous and cautions against any simple interpretation.“ 35s 6.1118, 2.2. .91-is, Chapter 8e 36. Of all the various consequences of social mobility, mental illness is perhaps the most prevalent. See, for example, A. B. Hollingshead, R. Ellis and 3. Kirby, "Social lobility and Rental Illness,” American Sociolc ical Review, 19 (October, 1954), 577-584, Evelyn Ellis, "Social Psychological Correlates of Upward Social Mobility Among Unmarried Career Women," American Sociological Revigz, 17 (October, 1952), 558-563. e f .-.-.l. I. 1! c . a ‘3 D e t . 0 1r 0 BIBLIOGRAPHY Angell, Robert C. "Preferences for Moral Norms in Three Areas," American Journal of Sociology, 67 (Hay, 1962), 650-660. Becker, Howard S. The Outsiders. New York: The Free Press, 1963. Bell, Wendell. ”Anomie, Social Isolation, and the Class Structure,” Sociomotry, 20 (June, 1957), 105-116. Benoit-Smullyan, Emile. ”Status, Status Types, and Status Inter-relations,“ American Sociological Review, 9 (April, 1944). 151-161. Blau, Peter. "Social lobility and Interpersonal Relations," American Sociological Review, 21 (June, 1956), 290—295. Bogardus, Emory. ”Race Reactions by Sex," Sociology and Social Research, 43 (July-August, 1959), 439.441. Bordua, David J. "Juvenile Delinquency and 'Anomie': An Attempt at Replication," Social Problems, 6 (Winter, 1958), 230-238. "'""""‘""""""'""’ Brymer, Richard A., and Sjoberg, Gideon. ”Bureaucracy, Community and the Lower Class.” Ianuscript in preparation. Cantril, Hadley. ”Hopes and Pears for Self and Country,” Americgg Behavioral Scigntist, 6 (October, 1962), Supplement. Cartwright, Desmond S. “A Iisapplication of Factor Analysis," American Sociological Review, 30 (April, 1965), 249-252. Clark, John P. "Measuring Alienation Within a Social System,” hgrican Sgciologicgl song, 24(Deoember, 1959), 849-852. Cloward, Richard A. "Illegitimate leans, Anomie, Deviant Behavior," ggeriogg Sociological Review, 24 (April, 1959), e and Ohlin, L. I. leinguenoy and Oppgrtunity. New York: The Pres Press, 19 0. Cohen, Albert R. "The Sociology of the Deviant Act," Americ Sociolc c 1 Rev ew, 39 (February, 1965), 5 (footnote 1). 248 z‘l -. .. .-. - a 5% x I ...” t I " 0 e ‘ i 0 Q. a G 0‘. e I. e . C.‘ -. e . Y K --.o~ ...-