....:.. .. . Z... ......T.... 1......3... .2 .....‘u. ...-... .....- ............ .....H............ ......._-...~fl_v..._.. . . . . ...-.... 2.2.... ... 1.... .1..- . 1.... ...: T... -..-.. ......._ .. 7... .. _. ........ . . xx. ... .37.}. ........ .. ............. .:.:.......1_2... 33...}..11 ...- .....:.. .....‘2..- ..........::..:. 37.....3...) ......... .. ......... .. 3....-. . ..\....._:. ...... .................. ................... ...s. .. 1.. .. . ...z . . . . .. .. .. 1...... .._,_.....,r .......r..._.. ..«nu..r§.|..w...x. ...}.F.......~:,a.. ....»th .13..” I... ... ...: . Mani... .....3: 5...... .... E ....._....... ... .....r...._:.: ........r............... ...... .... r : .. ... M.~.....r.... .72... ...-... . m ...- L is: A g y Michigan Sm: University This is to certify that the thesis entitled OUTCOME EFFECTS OF TRAINING COLLEGE STUDENT CLIENTS IN FACILITATIVE SELF-RESPONDING presented by David Walter Cabush has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for Ph.D. degreein Counseling, Personnel Services and Educational Psychology Date May IA, 1971 0-7639 £555 1‘! 43 25W. ABSTRACT OUTCOME EFFECTS OF TRAINING COLLEGE STUDENT CLIENTS IN FACILITATIVE SELF-RESPONDING BY David Walter Cabush The purpose of this experiment was to examine two fundamental questions: (1) Can clients be trained to respond to their own problems at minimally facilitative levels of empathy (E), regard (R), genuineness (G), and concreteness (C)? And, (2) does such training in "facili- tative self-reSponding" have a therapeutic effect in terms of client interpersonal functioning and behavior change in the direction of the individual's counseling goals? To seek answers for these questions two treatment conditions were established with the intent of making systematic comparisons of outcome. Twenty—two subjects were randomly assigned to one of the two treatment con- ditions, or groups. The subjects were college students who were seeking individual counseling for personal-social concerns and who had volunteered to participate. Subjects in Group 1 received individual counseling integrated with Systematic training in facilitative self—responding. David Walter Cabush Subjects in Group 2 received individual counseling with no systematic training. Both groups received treatment for one hour a week over a six-week period. Four counselors provided the treatment for Group 1, while five other counselors provided the treatment for Group 2. All nine counselors were judged equivalent on the dimensions of helper functioning using the Carkhuff scales for E, R, G, and C. Counselors were also deemed equivalent on the basis of experience, level of training, sex, age, and general therapeutic orientation. The procedure used for training in facilitative self—responding was developed by the researcher from pre- vious work that Carkhuff and associates had done in regard to facilitative interpersonal functioning. Hence, the procedure was an extension of the Carkhuff model and em- ployed modeling, didactic, and experiential sources of learning to shape higher levels of E, R, G, and C in self- help responses. Training followed two sequential phases: (1) training in the discrimination of levels of each of the facilitative dimensions, and (2) training in the com— munication of increasingly higher levels of each of the dimensions in response to the trainee's problems. To directly measure posttreatment training effec- tiveness subjects in Groups 1 and 2 were cast into the self-help role by being asked to respond as helper to three problem statements which were taken from a recording David Walter Cabush of their own intake interview. The responses were subse- quently rated by three independent judges on each of the facilitative dimensions. Reliability estimates for the ratings were .96 on E, .92 on R, .90 on G, and .94 on C. The results showed Group 1 to be functioning at about the 3.0 level on all dimensions and at about one level higher than Group 2. Univariate analyses of variance showed the differences to be statistically significant (a = .05) for each of the four measures. The Behavior Rating Form (BRF) was developed to measure group differences beyond the behaviors for which Group 1 was directly trained and behaviors generally accepted to be related to client benefit. Specifically, the BRF measured interpersonal functioning with items based upon the constructs of E, R, G, and C. Also, it measured behavior change in the direction of individual counseling goals with items based upon idiosyncratic problem behaviors identified by each subject prior to treatment. The BRF was administered to each subject and the two significant other observers whom each had identi— fied prior to treatment. Reliability estimates for the interpersonal dimension were .88 for observers and .79 for subjects, and for the counseling goals dimension were .80 for observers and .49 for subjects. Univariate analyses of variance showed Group 1 to be significantly better (a = .05) than Group 2 on the dimensions of interpersonal David Walter Cabush functioning and behavior change toward counseling goals as reported by observers, but not as reported by subjects. The differences reported by observers were not only sig— nificant but were meaningful. That is, according to ob- servers the behavior of Group 1 was in the desired direc— tion and that of Group 2 was not. Also, though group differences for subject ratings were not significant, they were in the direction favoring Group 1. In addition to the univariate analyses, overall treatment differences were assessed by considering all eight dependent measures together in a repeated measures analysis of variance. The results showed that Group 1 did significantly better (a = .05) than Group 2, but also yielded a significant ordinal interaction for groups and measures. In conclusion, facilitative self—responding was considered to be an effective therapeutic intervention. systematically training a client to respond to his own problems in a more facilitative manner, thus functioning more effectively as his own helper, is a promising direction for further exploration and development within the helping professions. OUTCOME EFFECTS OF TRAINING COLLEGE STUDENT CLIENTS IN FACILITATIVE SELF-RESPONDING BY David Walter Cabush A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Counseling, Personnel Services and Educational Psychology 1971 Copyright by DAVID WALTER CABUSH 1971 TO ROCHELLE, Joey, Cathy, and family ii ACKNOWLEDGMENT S Several very "significant others" contributed to making this study possible. Their brief mention here can scarcely convey the writer's deep feelings of appreciation. My guidance committee were co—workers in the most positive sense. Richard G. Johnson served as chairman, making many helpful contributions and yet allowing me to experience all that it meant for this to be "my study." (Zecil L. Williams served as research sponsor within the (Sounseling Center and never allowed me to feel that this 'was "just another dissertation." Andrew C. Porter's exper— tise in design and statistics was invaluable, and his emphasis upon meaningful research will not be forgotten. And Andrew M. Barclay ably served as representative from the Psychology Department. Richard Pierce contributed immeasurably in the early, but all important conceptual stages. He was in every sense "facilitative." Many Counseling Center staff cooperated in carrying out the experimental procedures. Without their help the various parts could not have come together successfully. iii Several fellow interns served as counselors and generously gave of their time, energy, and most importantly of themselves. For contributing to both the research and the researcher I thank Jim Archer, Bob Dendy, Sam Dietzel, Tom Fiester, Ken Hall, Tom Spierling, Bob VanNoord, and Tom Zarle. Mary Heiserman, Kathy Scharf, and Tom Zarle served as raters. They turned all that had been previously in— vested into that most important data. Kathy also served as "special consultant," and will continue a very special friend. Lee Erlandson served as my internship supervisor and was a genuine colleague. What I gained from our relationship will remain a vital part of me, and my ther- apy. In the terms of this study, he was truly a helper's helper. To Bill and Dinny Kell . . . thank you for defin- ing training as growing, and for providing the quality of nurture that allows adults to grow. And Bill, your "ESP" will never be forgotten. I now know that to get "stuck" is fundamentally human, but growth doesn't let one stay there. And then there is "Kelley" . . . subject, client, friend . . . unique, and yet representative of iv 'those with whom I was privileged to share the adventure, the joy, the pain, and the love that can be therapy. Finally, to Rochelle, my helpmate, my wife. Her lanelfiSh love and giving create feelings for which there eare no "concrete" labels or Operational measures, neverthe- less, they will be expressed throughout our future together. TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES . . . . . . . LIST OF FIGURES. . . . . . . . . Chapter I. RATIONALE . . . . . . . . Introduction. . . . . . . Purpose . . . . . . . . Theory and Supportive Research. Helping as an Interpersonal Process Facilitative Interpersonal Functioning Facilitative Dimensions and Their Levels. . . . . . . Training on the Facilitation Dimensions . . . . . Self-Directed Facilitative Conditions. II. METHODOLOGY. . . . . . . . Overview . . . . . . . . Sample. . . . . . . . . Counselors . . . . . . . Selection Rationale. . . . Counselor Training . . . . Subject Assignment . . . . Materials. . . . . . . . Measures . . . . . . . . vi Page ix wNI-J 004:. 16 20 24 24 26 28 28 32 32 33 37 Chapter Page Facilitative Level of Self-Help Responses . . . . . . . . . . 37 Behavior Rating Form . . . . . . . 41 Interpersonal Functioning. . . . . 41 Counseling Goals. . . . . . . . 43 Treatment Procedures. . . . . . . . 47 Experimental Group--Rationale . . . . 47 Experimental Group Format . . . . . 48 Stage I--Introduction (session 1) . . 48 Stage II-—Discrimination Training (sessions 1-3). . . . . . . . 49 Stage III--Communication Training (sessions 4-6). . . . . . . . 50 Control Group . . . . . . . . . 51 Treatment After Experimental Period . . 51 Hypotheses . . . . . . . . . . . 51 Experimental Design . . . . . . . . 54 Analysis of Data . . . . . . . . . 55 III. RESULTS. 0 O O O C O O O O O O O 56 Treatment Effects. . . . . . . . . 56 Interaction Effect . . . . . . . . 59 Univariate Effects . . . . . . . . 61 Facilitative Level of Self—Help Responses . . . . . . . . . . 61 Interpersonal Functioning . . . . . 65 Change Toward Individual Counseling Goals 0 O O O O O O O O O O 66 Subject Mortality. . . . . . . . 70 Status of Research Hypotheses. . . . . 72 IV. DISCUSSION. 0 O O O O O O O O O O 73 Overview. . . . . . . . . . . . 73 Limitations. . . . . . . . . . 76 Conclusions and Implications . . . . . 79 LIST OF REFERENCES . . . . . . . . . . . 89 vii Page APPENDICES Appendix A. Carkhuff Scales on Facilitative Dimensions . 94 B. Letter to Screening Counselors . . . . . 104 C. Procedural Guidelines for Screening Counselors . . . . . . . . . . . 105 D. Standard Letter of Explanation to Prospective Subjects. . . . . . . . 106 E. Subject Consent Form . . . . . . . . 108 F. "Part I"-—Pre-EXperimenta1 Data from Subject . . . . . . . . . . . . 109 G. Counselor Guidelines for Training in Facilitative Self—Responding . . . . . 111 H. Typescript of Standard Helpee-Helper Stimulus Tape . . . . . . . . . . 120 I. "Basic Dimensions of the Helping Process"—— Simplified Scales on the Facilitative Dimensions . . . . . . . . . . . 140 J. Letter to Significant Other Observers. . . 146 K. Instructions for Behavior Rating Form-— subject 0 O O O O O O O O C O O 147 L. Instructions for Behavior Rating Form—— Observers . . . . . . . . . . . 148 M. Items on Behavior Rating Form Based on Idiosyncratic Counseling Goals . . . . 149 N. Behavior Rating Form--Example . . . . . 153 0. Standard Instructions for Posttesting on Self—Help ReSponses . . . . . . . . 154 P. Rating Form for Tape Ratings. . . . . . 155 Q. Raw Scores on Dependent Measures According to Subject, Counselor, and Treatment Group. . . . . . . . . . . . . 156 viii LIST OF TABLES Table Page 1. Mean Level of Functioning on Facilitative Dimensions by Counselors With Subjects . . 30 2. Counselor Case Loads and Sex Distribution Within Treatment Groups . . . . . . . 33 3. Hoyt Reliability Coefficients for Ratings of Self-Help Responses on Facilitative Dimensions . . . . . . . . . . . 4O 4. Interpersonal Items on Behavior Rating Form With Self and Observer Reliability Coefficients. . . . . . . . . . . 44 5. Self and Observer Reliability Coefficients for Ratings on Idiosyncratic Goal Items. . 45 6. Mean Scores on Dependent Measures for Experimental and Control Treatment Groups . 57 7. Summary of the Repeated Measures Analysis of Variance . . . . . . . . . . . 58 8. Summary of Univariate Analyses of Variance . 62 9. Mean Level of Self-Help Responses on Facilitative Dimensions With Group Differentials (Carkhuff Scales) . . . . 65 10. Behavior Rating Form Sub Scale Reliability Coefficients for Subjects and Observers. . 67 11. Raw Score Means From Behavior Rating Form. . 69 12. Final Subject and Counselor Distribution Within Treatment Groups . . . . . . . 71 13. Raw Scores on Dependent Measures According to Subject, Counselor, and Treatment Group. . 156 ix LIST OF FIGURES Figure Page 1. Pictorial Representation of Posttest Tape for Measuring Facilitative Level of Self-Help Responses . . . . . . . . . 39 2. Pictorial Representation of Experimental DeSign O O O O O O O O O O O O O 55 3. Graph of Interaction Effect Between Groups * and Measures . . . . . . . . . . . 60 4. Illustration of Behavior Rating Form Response Format in Relation to Group Means on Measures 5 Through 8. . . . . . . . . 69 C HAPT ER I RAT IONALE Introduction Many professional helpers have advocated or implied that one of the results of successful treatment be greater independence or autonomy for the helpee. Krumboltz (1967) is representative of those who have more formally proposed that a primary function of counseling and counseling re- Search be the discovery of improved ways of helping clients prevent or learn to solve their own problems. If one assumes that a person seeking counseling is in fact manifesting his inability to resolve some problems independently, then it follows that a central endeavor of Counseling should be an improvement in the client's ability to help himself. To implement such a goal suggests that there is (1) an underlying problem solving or helping process that is applicable across a range of problems, and (2) that this process is amenable to predictable change. In recent years an impressive body of research on such a helping process has been generated by Carkhuff and his; associates. Their paradigm for "facilitative inter— pearsonal functioning" has provided both a descriptive and a tzraining model for improving the functioning of helpers. Stuzh training has been successfully implemented for pro— ikessional and paraprofessional helpers, significant others, arud helpees in terms of their interpersonal skills (Cark- hllff, 1969a, 1969b). However, prior to the present study tliere has not been a systematic attempt to use this model tc> train an individual client to function effectively as hi 5 own helper . Purpose The purpose of this experiment was to determine tine therapeutic effect of extending the Carkhuff model in Stuch a manner so as to train an individual client to :fiinction as his own helper. Specifically, this meant iJuiividually training clients to assume a helping role with themselves and to respond at higher levels on the facilitative dimensions of (l) empathy, (2) regard, (3) genuineness, and (4) concreteness.* Effectiveness was evaluated by comparing counseled clients who received systematic training in "facilitative self-responding" over a six—week period to counseled clients who did not receive training over the same time *To be abbreviated as follows: E (empathy), R (regard), G (genuineness), and C (concreteness). period. Comparisons were made on outcome criteria which were judged to be relevant and feasible. (1) The level of E, R, G, and C in the responses which a client made after treatment to his problems as he was verbalizing them prior to treatment; (2) The client's interpersonal functioning in terms of the communication of E, R, G, and C to others as reported by the client and significant others; (3) Change on the idiosyncratic problem—related be— haviors which the client could identify when he entered counseling as reported by the client and significant others. Using these criteria for comparing counseled clients who received training to those who did not, it was hypothe— sized that: 1. When cast in the role of being their own helper counseled clients receiving training will score significantly higher on the levels of E, R, G, and C than will counseled clients receiving no training. 2. Counseled clients receiving training will score significantly higher in terms of the communication of E, R, G, and C to others as reported by the client and significant others than will the counseled clients receiving no training. 3. Counseled clients receiving training will show significantly greater positive change on idio— syncratic problem-related behaviors as reported by the client and significant others when com- pared to counseled clients receiving no training. Theory and Supportive Research Because this study is an extension of what is an already well elaborated and widely researched paradigm-- "facilitative interpersonal functioning"—-the researcher Will focus upon its theoretical base and related research. This model is essentially eclectic——incorporating didactic, experiential, and modeling means for acquiring or changing behavior. Consequently, the underlying theory has elements in common with most of the major personality theories, and the methods of therapeutic intervention share elements with most schools of psychotherapy. Such eclecticism has often been criticized for logical inconsistencies or heuristic limitations. However, even cursory review of the work of Carkhuff and associates suggests that their paradigm does "hang together" theo- retically, and that it is rich in both accomplished and potential research. But of greatest importance to this researcher is the degree to which it translates into demonstrable human benefits. This would seem to be the most valid criterion for any theory or model of helping. Helping as an Interpersonal Process The model for facilitative interpersonal function— ing rests upon a fundamental assumption. Assumption: Helping processes and their training programs are all instances of interpersonal learning or relearning (Carkhuff, 1969b, p. 3). Virtually every personality theorist has attended t0 the nature of early interpersonal relationships. The QUality of these relationships are seen as at least Partially predictive of adult psychological functioning. From early childhood an individual's emotional, intel- lectual, and physical development can be viewed as having been significantly dependent upon: (1) a "first person" (e.g., parents, teachers, counselors); (2) a "second person" (e.g., child, student, client); and (3) "contextual vari— ables" (e.g., environmental factors, including conditions offered by others). These first person and second person roles also become defined as more knowing/less knowing, and helper/helpee (Carkhuff, 1969a). The issue of adequacy or quality of these inter— personal processes, or helper—helpee relationships, lead to some central propositions. Proposition I: The helping process in any inter— personal setting may have constructive or deteriorative consequences. The first published challenge to the effectiveness of professional helping came from Eysenck (1952). Since then many researchers have taken a closer look at counsel— ing outcomes. The result has been that a significant in— crease of both constructive and deteriorative consequences has been noted on a variety of indices in treatment groups When compared to controls. Such has been the case in pro— grams of intensive treatment with hospitalized schizo— Ehrenics (Rogers, Gendlin, Kiesler, & Truax, 1967; Truax & Carkhuff, 1967), with outpatient neurotics (Barron & Leary, 1955; Cartwright & Vogel, 1960), and with relatively nonpathological guidance populations (Mink & Isaksen, 1959). The evidence indicates that even when the statistics yield no mean differences on outcome measures, the treatment group variance is greater than the control group variance. Therefore, the treatment group has increased positive and negative scores which indicate that helpers do have an impact-~for better or for worse. Evidence of constructive and deteriorative conse— quences also exists for other important interpersonal relationships. Carkhuff (1969b) has reviewed the growing body of literature concerning parent-child, teacher— student, and helper trainer—trainee relationships. The interpersonal learning within these relationships has been linked with a wide range of consequences, including social adjustment, manifestation of hostility, emergence of SChizophrenia, academic achievement, and level of effec— tiVeness as a professional helper. Thus, our knowledge to this point indicates that within a variety of interpersonal settings helping relation— Ships can be facilitative or retarding. Consequently, much research has assumed the approach of looking at the outcome and tracing back through the process in an effort to deter— mine the variables which promote, inhibit, or malign growth. The following proposition has grown out of this approach and it is fundamental to the Carkhuff model. PrOposition II: "All effective interpersonal processes share a common core of conditions conducive to facilitate human experiences" (Carkhuff, 1969b, p. 7). Rogers (1957) delineated six conditions which were posited as "necessary to initiate constructive personality change." 1. Two persons are in psychological contact. 2. The first . . . , the client, is in a state of incongruence, being vulnerable or anxious. 3. The second . . . , the therapist, is congruent or integrated in the relationship. 4. The therapist experiences unconditional positive regard for the client. 5. The therapist experiences an empathic understand— ing of the client's internal frame of reference and endeavors to communicate this experience to the client. 6. The communication to the client of the thera- pist's empathic understanding is to a minimal degree achieved (pp. 95—96). Other widely divergent orientations to counseling and psychotherapy have subsequently emphasized the critical nature of the helper and helpee process variables. Volumes Could be written on the subject of these variables and related research. A brief review serves to provide a Zperspective for the variables central to this experiment. Though sometimes given different labels, helper ‘Variables have included: (1) empathic understanding, (2) Exositive regard or warmth, (3) genuineness, (4) self— disclosure, (5) specificity of emotional experience, (6) concreteness in problem solving, (7) appropriate confron— tation, and (8) interpretation of the immediate helping experience (Carkhuff, 1969b). Helpee variables have included: (1) self-exploration (Truax & Carkhuff, 1967), (2) owning of feeling, (3) commitment to change, (4) differentiation of stimuli, and (5) changing behavior with the helper (Kagan, et 31., 1967, 1969). Facilitative Interpersonal Functioning In what are essentially eclectic extensions of Rogers' (1957) "necessary and sufficient conditions," Carkhuff and associates (Carkhuff, 1969a, 1969b; Carkhuff & Berenson, 1967; Truax & Carkhuff, 1967) have added helper-initiated dimensions to the helper—responsive con— ditions. In this context, effective helping processes can be broken down into two general components: (1) under- standing, and (2) action. Within each component critical variables have been delineated. Understanding: (l) empathy, (2) regard, (3) genuineness, and (4) concreteness. Action: (1) confrontation and (2) immediacy. The understanding component is fundamental to the helping process and its variables are thus called "core facilitative dimensions." However, it should be noted that genuineness and concreteness have been considered "swing dimensions" in that according to the developmental phases of helping they may be either reSponsive or initi- ative. All four dimensions represent the "common core of conditions" referred to in Proposition II as "conducive to facilitate human experiences,‘ and consequently define the helping process as it is incorporated into this study. Most therapeutic models can be viewed as having at least two major interdependent phases which parallel the components of understanding and action: (1) the "in- ward" phase where the establishment of the relationship and helpee self-exploration are the major elements; and (2) the "outward" phase where directionality is established and specific behavior changes are often sought. In terms of the facilitative dimensions, during the initial phase the concentration is upon empathy and regard. High levels of E and R are posited as leading to feelings of being understood, acceptance, and self-understanding which in turn reduces anxiety related to what has been verbalized and thus is reinforcing for further self-exploration. During the later phase the focus is upon a genuine and sharing relationship where specific direction emerges and goals are implemented by concrete measures. Carkhuff and Berenson (1967) have summarized the process accordingly: (1) The therapist institutes high levels of facili- tative conditions which (2) enable the client to explore himself meaningfully and (3) sets the thera- pist up as a potent reinforcing agent who helps to give directionality to the client's struggle, and finally (4) translates into constructive action on the client's part (p. 142). Facilitative Dimensions and Their Levels At this juncture the concept of "level" is criti— cal. As Carkhuff and associates have defined the 10 dimensions of E, R, G, and C they are not some static quality that a helper mysteriously has, or has not. Rather, they can be seen as operational at some level in any helper-helpee interaction. For each dimension five levels have been defined, scaled from 1 through 5. Level has been defined as the "minimally facilitative level of interpersonal functioning" (Carkhuff & Berenson, 1967, pp. 5-7) for all dimensions. Levels 4 and 5 facilitate change in a positive direction while levels 2 and l in— hibit positive change or produce negative effects. Con- sidering the dimensions individually should help clarify the rationale used in constructing the scales as well as more thoroughly define the variables. (The actual scales, where each level is defined, can be found in Appendix A.) Empathy. At level 3 the verbal responses of the helper and the verbal expressions of the helpee are essen- tially interchangeable in that they express basically the same affect and meaning. Below level 3 the helper's responses do not attend to or significantly detract from the expressed feelings of the helpee. Low level responses often represent a distortion of the helpee's expression on the part of the helper. Above level 3 the helper's re— Sponses add noticeably to the expressions of the helpee in such a way as to express accurately feelings and mean- ings which the helpee was not able to verbalize. ll Regard. At level 3 the helper communicates a respect or positive regard and concern for the helpee's feelings, experiences, and potentials. Below level 3 the helper indicates a lack of regard. This may be communi— cated in a number of ways such as responding mechanically, passively, or ignoring the feelings of the helpee. Above level 3 the helper communicates a deep respect and caring for the helpee's feelings, experiences, and potentials as a worthwhile person. He enables the helpee to feel free to be himself and to experience being valued as an indi— vidual. Genuineness. At level 3 the helpee provides no discrepancies between what he verbalizes and what other cues indicate he is feeling, but also, there are no posi— tive cues to indicate a really personal and genuine re— 5ponse to the helpee. Below level 3 there are discrepan— cies indicated. The helper's verbalizations may be un— related to what he is feeling, or he may be Spontaneous and genuine with only his negative responses to the helpee. Above level 3 the helper provides some definite cues indi— cating a genuine response to the helpee in a non- destructive manner. The helper is open to what he him- self is experiencing and employs this constructively. Concreteness. At level 3 the helper enables the helpee to deal with personally relevant material in Specific and concrete terminology. Below level 3 various 12 levels of vagueness and abstractness dominate the verbal interaction. Above level 3 the helper enables the helpee to develop in concrete and specific terms most instances of concern. High levels are often characterized by con- crete problem solving activities. There have been efforts to analyze how these four dimensions might be related structurally. One such study (Muehlberg, Drasgow, & Pierce, 1969) used previous research data to designate one high, one moderate, and one low level functioning therapist. Audio tapes of therapy sessions were rated for E, R, G, C and self—disclosure, and the results factor analyzed. The results replicated the pre- vious levels of functioning of the three therapists, and the factor analysis yielded one principal factor for each therapist. The limitations of this study are obvious, the factors may well have been a function of the single thera- pists. A more carefully designed study (Berenson, Friel, & Mitchell, 1970) explored the relationship of factorial dimensions for just high— and low-functioning therapists. Initial interviews of 13 high level therapists and 32 low level therapists were rated for E, R, G, and C. The high level therapists provided a more complex factor structure involving three factors while the low level therapists provided a simple factor structure involving only one factor. Though the sample size was relatively small, 13 these results suggest that the processes provided by high— and low-functioning therapists not only differ functionally, but also structurally. One inference which might be made is that high-functioning therapists have a larger reper— toire of responses to offer clients. An issue related to the helper's reSponse reper— toire and level of functioning is how the type of affect and problem content of the helpee expressions relate to the helper's level of responding. To explore this question Carkhuff (1969c) developed an audio tape with 15 helpee stimulus statements. Three classifications of affect——(l) depression-distress, (2) anger—hostility, and (3) elation-excitement——were crossed and balanced with five problem content areas--(l) social-interpersonal, (2) educational—vocational, (3) child—rearing, (4) sexual— marital, and (5) confrontation of helper. Individuals from four sub—populations--(l) general, (2) college stu— dents, (3) lay personnel, and (4) professional helpers-— were cast into the helping role and asked to respond to the stimulus expressions. Their responses were rated on the facilitative dimensions. Factor analyses indicated that a single factor accounted for two—thirds of the variability which suggests that all of the tests were essentially measuring the same variable 15 times. This was obtained independent of affect, content, and experi- ence level. However, multivariate analyses for high-, moderate-, and low-functioning helpers (in terms of E, R, 14 G, and C) yielded significant differences in group means and dispersions. Yet, within these levels of functioning the differences between affect and content areas were slight and not meaningful. In summary: (1) respondents who were inexperienced and/or low functioning tended to respond to content almost exclusively, (2) respondents who were experienced and/or moderate functioning tended to respond to affect and content with equal frequency, and (3) respondents experienced and systematically trained and/or high functioning tended to respond to both affect and content simultaneously. One conclusion one might draw from this study is that it is characteristic of high level functioning helpers to integrate problem content and related affect in their responses to helpees. In Rogerian (1961) terms it might very well be the helpee‘s inability to achieve such an integration, or "congruence," independently that precipitates his seeking help. The issue of how E, R, G, and C are related structurally is complex and the research is incomplete. At this point there is no basis for claiming that they are necessarily independent of one another within the helping process. Factorially there is evidence that their relationship to one another, helpee affect, and problem content, differs according to the levels of the process. Of primary importance is that the four dimensions describe some critical aspects of the helping process 15 which has resulted in the formulation of a functional model of the same. Research indicates that independent of the theoretical orientation of helpers, these dimensions are related to constructive process and/or outcome within various interpersonal settings and with a range of helpees. Examples include schizophrenics, both those in individual (Berenson & Mitchell, 1969; Rogers et al., 1967; Truax & Carkhuff, 1967) and group psychotherapy (Truax, 1961; Truax, Carkhuff, & Kodman, 1965), and outpatient neurotics or situationally distressed populations (Barrett—Lennard, 1962; Berenson & Mitchell, 1969; Carkhuff & Berenson, 1967; Halkides, 1958; Pagell gt al., 1967; Truax & Carkhuff, 1967). There is also evidence that supports the propo— sition that these dimensions are related to learning in formal teaching situations, childrearing, and effective training programs in the helping professions. A review of these findings can be found in Carkhuff (1969b, pp. 8—9). In an attempt to gain an estimate of the levels of E, R, G, and C offered in the everyday environment to persons in need of help, Martin and Carkhuff (1967) cast individuals from the general public in the helping role and rated their level of functioning. The helpers' average level of functioning across the core dimensions was 1.5, with a range of 1.4 to 1.8. Carkhuff and Berenson (1967) have interpreted these results as follows: 16 . . . in our assessments of the general population, the first person, at a maximum, responds to the super— ficial feelings of the other person, not only infre- quently, but also continuing to ignore the deeper feelings; he communicates little positive regard, dis- playing a lack of concern or interest for the second person; his verbalizations are somewhat unrelated to what he is feeling, and most often he is responding according to a prescribed "role" rather than by ex- pressing what he personally feels or means; he fre- quently leads or allows discussions of material per- sonally relevant to the second person to be dealt with on a vague or abstract level (p. 8). Although the controlled research evidence is cursory, this study lends support to the premise that the levels of human "nourishment" available to the average individual at a point of psychological crisis are inade- quate. This condition offers further impetus to the search for methods of self-help. Thus far, it appears that at least some of the dimensions of effective helping have been identified and can be generalized across a range of interpersonal helping situations and helpee problems. This leads to the funda- mental issue as to whether these dimensions have been sufficiently operationalized so as to effect change on them through training procedures. Training on the Facilitative Dimensions Proposition III: Individuals can be trained to function interpersonally at minimally facili— tative levels of empathy (E), regard (R), genuineness (G), and concreteness (C). 17 Systematic training programs have been developed for improving the interpersonal functioning on the dimen- sions of E, R, G, and C. Research has shown that the ability to discern a high level (above 3.0) from a low level response is a prerequisite for, but not a guarantee of, the capacity to initiate a high level response (Cark- huff, Kratochvil, & Friel, 1968). This issue is not unlike the insight-action conflict of therapeutic processes. An individual may come to see (insight) but not act upon what he sees, perhaps having a dynamic base of having been con- ditioned to fear acting upon what he sees. Because of this phenomenon, effective training has incorporated two sequential phases--(l) discrimination training, and (2) communication training. Both phases have included the experiential, didactic, and modeling sources of learning for shaping the facilitative level of trainee responses. Training programs have followed three modalities—- (1) training helpers, (2) training significant others, and (3) training helpees directly. Training helpers. Carkhuff (1969a) has made a comprehensive review of helper training programs based upon the facilitative dimensions (pp. 152-157). Trainers have ranged from Ph.D. level psychologists to non-degree personnel with no formal psychological training. Trainees have ranged from doctoral students in clinical psychology to laymen from various non-professional backgrounds. He has summarized the results as follows: 18 Those trainees whose trainers were functioning above minimally facilitative levels and approximately one level or more above the trainees demonstrated the most positive changes (p. 155). Pierce and Schauble (1970) came to similar con- clusions after studying counselors in graduate training and their supervisors over a 30-week period. Interns having supervisors who were functioning at high levels of E, R, G, and C changed significantly and positively on these dimensions. Those having supervisors functioning at low levels on these dimensions did not change, in fact tended to decline slightly. Thus, when evaluating the training of helpers upon the basis of their resultant level of interpersonal functioning, the level of the trainers' functioning appears to be the most critical dimension of effectiveness. Training significant others. In situations where the helpee or client has repeated contact with particular individuals, training these significant others to be effective helpers is most logical. Often such signifi- cant others are in a position, in terms of time and quality of the relationship, to have an impact that could hardly be matched by professional helpers. Training programs for such significant others have been implemented for the parents of emotionally disturbed Children (Carkhuff & Bierman, 1970), teachers (Berenson, 1970; Bierman, Carkhuff, & Santilli, 1970; Carkhuff & Griffin, 1971), peers (Berenson, Carkhuff, & Myrus, 1966), 19 institutional attendants (Carkhuff & Truax, 1965a, 1965b), nurses (Kratochvil, 1968), guards (Carkhuff, Banks, Beren— son, Griffin, & Hall, 1971), and dormitory resident assist— appg (Sharf, 1971). The results indicate that systematic training of significant others has a positive affect upon both the client and his symbiont. Further, in the only research comparing experiential training with systematic training in experiential dimensions (Berenson, Carkhuff, & Myrus, 1966, using dormitory undergraduate counselors), and systematic training with experiential counseling (Carkhuff & Bierman, 1970, using parents of emotionally disturbed children), the results significantly favor systematic approaches. Direct training of helpees. The most direct form of any treatment would be the direct training of helpees in those skills necessary to function effectively. The particular skills necessary vary according to the problem of the helpee and the therapeutic orientation of the trainer. To remain within the scope of this study the focus is upon interpersonal skills, particularly those defined by the facilitative dimensions of E, R, G, and C. Assuming the necessity of such interpersonal Skills for getting and staying out of the hospital, Pierce and.Drasgow (1969) employed systematic training on the facilitative dimensions with a group of chronic neuro- PSychiatric patients who were not otherwise being treated. 20 Comparisons were made with four control groups receiving (1) treatment with drugs, (2) individual therapy, (3) group therapy, or (4) a combination of the three. The results were significantly in favor of the trained helpees, both in terms of gains in process variables and outpatient status. Vitalo (1970) has replicated these results with other psychiatric populations. Concerning the direct training of helpees, Cark- huff (197lb) has concluded the following: The most direct form of training as treatment, then, is to train the client himself in the skills which he needs to function effectively. The culmination of such a program is to train the client to develop his own training program. To say, "Client, heal thyself!" and to train him in the skills necessary to do so is not only the most direct—~but it is also the most honest and most effective--form of treatment known to man (p. 127). One mode of training which has not been researched to date is training the helpee to function as his own helper within the Carkhuff paradigm. If at least some of the dimensions of effective helping and/or functioning have been identified and even laymen can be trained to employ these as effective helpers, then a logical exten- sion seems to be training for effective helping with oneself. .§e1f-Directed Facilitative @nditions Extending the model for facilitative interpersonal functioning to a method of self—help implies that the 21 (dimensions of E, R, G, and C maintain important elements of their meaning, and consequently their effectiveness, when considered as self-directed. A brief overview of the dimensions so construed should help further establish the rationale for training in facilitative self-responding. Self-directed empathy. The degree to which an individual is able to discern and attend to those feelings which he is verbalizing in the context of his "problem," and the affects and meanings which are related but not verbalized. This seems closely related to the "owning of feeling" dimension described and scaled by Shauble and Pierce (1970). Self-directed empathy suggests an accurate sensitivity to one's subjective, affective self. Self-directed regard. The degree to which an individual cares about his feelings, experiences, and potentials. This may be viewed dynamically from various persPectives, ranging from ego-strength to self-concept. The essential factor seems to be that the individual cares sufficiently about himself so as to become adequately in— volved in some psychological growth process. The degree to which one can be trained systematically on this dimension is an open question, but it does appear that training might at least aid an individual in gaining an estimate of where he stands on this critical dimension. Self-directed genuineness. The degree to which one can be honest, spontaneous, and confronting with 22 himself. Being genuine relates to how well one can discern discrepancies and incongruities within his behavior reper- toire. Such awareness does not guarantee action, but would appear to increase one's options for constructive and non— conflicting behaviors. Self-directed concreteness. The degree to which one can deal with all instances of concern in specific and concrete terms. Being concrete with oneself means dealing with problem content and related affect in those terms which are most likely to be amenable to direct problem solving activities such as the development of specific skills which may be needed. When considering the facilitative dimensions as self-directed there is the tendency to label the process by changing the prefix from "inter" to intra" in the Carkhuff nomenclature. But, this would be deceptive to the extent that "intra" connotes purely private events. The training model used was in fact an extension of that which has been found to be effective interpersonally and, therefore, was expected to replicate the positive results in terms of interpersonal functioning. However, there were three unique factors within this training procedure: (1) during the discrimination phase of training, self- help reSponses were modeled rather than other—helper re- sponses; (2) during the communication phase of training Self-directed helping responses were practiced in addition 23 to other-directed responses; and (3) the psychological set and expectations of each counselor-trainer was different to the extent that the goal was perceived as shaping self- help responses. (The complete training procedure will be detailed in Chapter II.) In conclusion, the rationale for this study has three major elements: (1) the development to date of the Carkhuff model for facilitative functioning, (2) the mounting evidence for training as an effective and even preferred mode of treatment, and (3) the current emphasis from various sectors of the helping professions upon pre- paring their clients to resolve their own immediate and future problems. Fundamentally, the experiment tests the proposition that an individual can be trained to become a higher level functioning helper with himself by employing higher levels of empathy, regard, genuineness, and con- creteness when dealing with his problems and concerns, and that such training makes a positive difference in his interpersonal functioning and the resolution of the idio- syncratic problems which motivated him to seek counseling. In humanistic terms, what has been attempted is to place the tools of helping into the hands of the person asking for help . . . and training him to use them. CHAPTER II METHODOLOGY Overview Twenty—two undergraduate college students who were seeking individual counseling for personal-social concerns volunteered to participate in a "six-week counsel- ing research project." They were randomly assigned to one of two treatment groups. One was the experimental treat— ment group where they received individual counseling inte— grated with systematic training in facilitative self- reSponding. The second was the treatment control group where they received individual counseling with no system— atic training. Both the experimental and control subjects received treatment for one hour a week over a six-week period. The systematic training procedure used with the experimental group was developed by the researcher and was an extension of the Carkhuff model for training in facili- tative interpersonal functioning. The Carkhuff model was modified so as to focus upon training subjects to respond at higher levels of empathy (E), regard (R), genuineness (G), and concreteness (C) to their own problems. For 24 25 experimental subjects this training accounted for approxi- mately two-thirds of their counseling time. Nine counselors participated in the study, four in the experimental group and five in the control group. All nine counselors were judged equivalent on the levels of interpersonal functioning using the Carkhuff scales for E, R, G, and C. The counselors were also deemed equiva- lent on the bases of experience, level of training, sex, and therapeutic orientation. Prior to training, subjects were asked to identify behaviors which they considered related to their problems, the changing of which would constitute counseling "goals." Also, subjects were asked for the names of two acquaint— ances who would be in a position to observe their behavior over the ensuing six—week period. The Behavior Rating Form was constructed for the purpose of measuring the interpersonal functioning of the subjects and changes on the individual goals for counsel- ing. The interpersonal items were developed upon the con- structs of E, R, G, and C. The same items were used across all subjects. The goal items were taken from those de- scribed by the subjects prior to treatment and were thus unique for each subject. Following treatment the Behavior Rating Form was administered to the significant others by mail, and to the subjects in person. The instrument so administered 26 generated four dependent measures: significant—other obser- ver ratings on (1) interpersonal behavior and (2) behavior change in the direction of goals; self-ratings on (3) interpersonal behavior and (4) behavior change in the direction of goals. So that posttreatment training effectiveness might be measured directly, subjects in Group 1 (experimental treatment) and Group 2 (treatment-control) were cast into the self—help role by being asked to reSpond as helper to three problem statements which had been excerpted (see p. 38) from their own recorded intake interview. These responses were rated by three independent judges on the dimensions of E, R, G, and C. The ratings generated four additional dependent measures respectively. A repeated measures analysis of variance was per- formed to test for a group main effect and group with measures interaction. Univariate ANOVA's were performed on each of the eight dependent measures. Reliability estimates were computed for each of the dependent measures. Sample It was not feasible within this study to obtain a random sample from the general population of interest. Therefore, the sample and sampling procedure has been carefully delineated, allowing the reader to judge how this study's population compares with some population to Which he might wish to generalize. 27 A sample of 22 subjects was obtained over the first three weeks of winter term, 1971, at the Michigan State University Counseling Center. According to Counseling Center policy, students seeking counseling are seen the same day by a screening counselor whose function it is to make an assessment of the problem(s) and discuss possible courses of action with the student. Eleven such counselors agreed to participate in generating the sample. After eXploring a student's concern a given screening counselor was able to identify a prospective subject according to provided guidelines (see Appendix C). Students so identi- fied were introduced to the study as one alternative for receiving help. They were shown a standard letter of explanation (see Appendix D) which essentially stated that the purpose of the research was to compare counseling approaches, that the study would be for six weeks, and pre and post responsibilities were explained. In no manner was facilitative self—responding discussed. On the basis of this information each student made a choice about volunteering for the study. Screening counselors reported that of those students who met the criteria and were pre- sented with the letter of explanation, only two declined to participate. An audio recording was made of every screening interview. No student objected to this procedure when asked prior to the interview. These recordings were used 28 to construct a posttest which will be discussed in the section entitled "Measures." Sample Characteristics 1. Sex: 9 males, 13 females. 2. Age: range = 19-24 years, mean = 20.8 years. 3. Class standing at Michigan State University: 12 sophomores, 8 juniors, and 2 seniors. 4. Came to M.S.U. Counseling Center seeking individual counseling, winter term, 1971. 5. Had a screening interview where client and counselor agreed that the general classifi- cation of the problem was "personal-social," as differentiated from educational, vocational, or administrative. 6. Presenting problem was not judged by screening counselor to be "urgent" or of a crisis nature. 7. Not a substantial history of psychopathology, e.g., psychiatric hospitalization or previous long-term professional psychological help. 8. Those responding positively when asked to participate in the study. Counselors Selection Rationale Nine counselors were available and willing to participate in the study. Random assignment of counselors to either experimental or control treatment conditions was not possible. Though randomization was the preferred method of controlling for individual counselor effects, there were alternate bases for assuming the nine counselors to be equivalent on dimensions relevant to counselor 29 functioning. In accord with the research and rationale noted in Chapter I, it was concluded that the primary dimensions of concern were the levels of helper empathy (E), regard (R), genuineness (G), and concreteness (C). Prior to the treatment period the researcher requested an audio tape of a counseling session from each of the counselors tentatively chosen for the study. These tapes were rated by the researcher on the dimensions of E, R, G, and C. The decision to retain the counselor for the study was based upon whether his modal level of functioning on all dimensions was 3.0 or above. All nine counselors were judged to be functioning at 3.0 or above, that is, they were functioning at minimally facilitative levels. This preliminary assessment of counselor function- ing did not directly measure the counselors' level of functioning with the subjects in the study. For this purpose the fifth treatment sessions was randomly selected for rating on the dimensions of E, R, G, and C. Three 4-minute segments-—first 4, middle 4, and last 4 minutes—— were excerpted from every counselor’s fifth session with every subject. Rating was done by three independent raters (see p. 40) for further discussion of raters). Because the counselors were initially selected on the basis of equivalent levels of functioning, the variance of their ratings was small-~smaller differences than the 30 Carkhuff scales were designed to discriminate. This limited range of ratings precluded conventional relia- bility estimates. However, the same raters were known to be reliable from other ratings where estimates were possible (p. 40). TABLE 1 Mean Level of Functioning on Facilitative Dimensions by Counselors With Subjects Facilitative Dimensions Counselors Empathy Regard Genuineness Concreteness Experimental 1 3.56 3.56 3.50 3.50 2 3.56 3.33 3.17 3.72 3 3.44 3.17 3.17 3.39 4 3.39 3.22 3.17 3.50 Group Mean 21:13. 1.23 112.5. .322. Control 5 3.22 3.17 3.17 3.50 6 3.33 3.33 3.39 3.50 7 3.39 3.33 3.39 3.33 8 3.11 3 28 3.22 3.06 9 3.22 3.33 3.28 3.28 Group Mean 3.27 3.31 3.39 3.33 31 The results of the tape ratings confirm that all nine counselors were functioning at minimally facilitative levels of E, R, G, and C. The slight differences which exist are not meaningful in terms of how the dimensions are defined. Therefore, there is a sound basis for assum— ing that the counselors were essentially equivalent in terms of their facilitative level of functioning with the subjects in this experiment. Though the level of functioning is of primary im- portance, there was additional support for assuming counse- lor equivalence across experimental and control conditions. All nine were males, ranging in age from 28 to 35 years for the experimental group, and 27 to 34 years for the control group. All were doctoral level students at Michigan State University having completed at least 80 per cent of their academic work. They were first-year interns in the Coun— seling Center, the selection process for which tends to reduce differences by selecting from the applicants a limited number who are considered most qualified on the basis of diverse criteria. The internship itself is a training program where trainees have many experiences in common, both experiential and didactic. In terms of their general therapeutic orientation all the counselors acknowl— edged an emphasis upon interpersonal dynamics and their developmental antecedents, and none of the counselors identify primarily with the behavior modification model. 32 Therefore, on the bases of (1) level of facilitative functioning, (2) sex, (3) age, (4) level of experience, (5) level of training, and (6) general therapeutic orien- tation, there is strong evidence that counselors were a highly homogeneous group and that differential treatment effects could be assumed attributable to other factors. Counselor Training All nine counselors were experienced with various training methodologies, particularly those dealing with paraprofessionals. The experimental counselors had one 90-minute session together where the counselor guidelines (see Appendix G) were reviewed along with the SHeHrS tape (described on p. 35). They did not receive pre-treatment experiential training in facilitative self—responding. Though such training may have Optimized trainer effective— ness, it was considered too great a threat to the internal validity of the experiment. That is, experiential train— ing for Group 1 counselors may have altered their estab- lished equivalence with Group 2 counselors. The other alternative was to provide training for all counselors, but this was impractical within the limits of this study and could have contaminated the control treatment. §Bbject Assignment The 22 subjects were randomly assigned to either the experimental or control group. They were then assigned 33 to their individual counselor according to scheduling and case load practicalities. All counselors were supplied with a standard student folder containing academic records, demographic data, and notes written by the screening counselor. TABLE 2 Counselor Case Loads and Sex Distribution Within Treatment Groups Sex of Subjects Counselors Number of Subjects Females Males Experimental 1 5 3 2 2 2 l 1 3 2 l l 4 .1 1 1 Totals 11 6 5 Control 5 3 l 2 6 2 2 O 7 2 2 0 8 2 l l 9 .1 1 1 Totals 11 7 4 Materials Various materials were required for particular Phases of the study and were Obtained or developed by the researcher. 34 Counselor pre-tapes. All nine counselors tenta- tively chosen to participate in the study were asked to supply an audio recording of one counseling session. These were rated by the researcher to assure that all counselors were functioning at minimally facilitative levels. Selection and use of screening counselors. Obtain— ing the cooperation of screeners and equipping them for obtaining subjects involved the following materials: 1. An open letter to screening counselors seek— ing their cooperation in obtaining the sample (see Appendix B). 2. "Procedures for Screening Counselor" (see Appendix C). 3. Letter explaining the study to prospective subjects (see Appendix D). 4. "Consent Form" to be signed by those agreeing to become subjects (see Appendix E). Pre-experimental information from subjects. "Part I" was given to subjects by their screening counselor immediately after screening. This provided two types of Pre-experimental information needed from subjects (see Appendix F). l. Behaviors they could identify as related to their problems, the changing of which would constitute counseling goals. This information was for developing the posttest Behavior Rating Form and was not made available to counselors. 2. 35 The names of two significant-others who could function as observers and complete the Behavior Rating Form after the treatment period. Experimental treatment materials. Special materials were used at different stages of the training in facili— tative self-responding. l. The Standard Helpee-Helper Stimulus tape (SHeHrS) was an audio tape recording contain- ing 13 assorted helpee problem statements, each followed by three self-help responses. The self—help responses were designed so that one of them was at least minimally facilitative (3.0 or above) and at least one level (Cark- huff, l969a) higher than the others. This section of the tape was used for discrimination training. Following these were 11 assorted helpee statements without responses. Subjects were asked to respond as helpers to these, thus constituting communication training. All of the statements and responses were record— ings of role-playing by non-professional actors. A typescript of the SHeHrS tape was made available for the experimental group counselOrs (see Appendix H). The Carkhuff 5-point scales for measuring the levels of E, R, G, and C were revised to 36 obtain 3-point scales. Concrete examples were written for each level of the four dimen- sions. These revised scales were incorporated into a handout entitled "Basic Dimensions of the Helping Process" (see Appendix I) which was made available for scheduled presentation to the training subjects. 3. The original Carkhuff 5-point scales were made available to counselors in the experimental group for their further understanding of the dimensions and their levels (see Appendix A). 4. Guidelines were written and copies given to counselors doing the training in facilitative self-responding (see Appendix G). 5. An audio tape recorder was provided for train— ing sessions in addition to the one used in recording the sessions. This machine was used for the SHeHrS tape, and for recording and playback of scheduled role-play exercises. No special materials were required for the control counseling sessions other than an audio tape recorder and Sufficient tapes for recording all sessions. Letter to significant-other observers. In order to obtain the cooperation of the observers in completing the Behavior Rating Form, an explanatory letter accompanied the instrument. Both were mailed after the subjects had completed the sixth session (see Appendix J). 37 Measures There were essentially two modes of measurement used to compare treatment outcomes. One sought to answer the question: "Can subjects be trained to respond to their own problems at minimally facilitative level of E, R, G, and C?" The second sought to answer the question: "Does training in facilitative self-responding make a significant and meaningful difference on dimensions re— 1ated to subject benefit?" Facilitative Level of Self- Help Resppnses To measure the posttreatment facilitative level of self-help responses, every subject was cast into the helping role and asked to respond to his own problem state- ments. This was considered the most direct and valid measure of the behavior of interest. Three problem statements were used for each subject. The statements were excerpted from the recorded screening interviews, thus sampling directly from how the subjects were verbalizing concerns at the time they were seeking help. This is an important point in terms of construct validity. Theoretically, a given subject is seeking a Inxafessional helper because he is not able to obtain mininml facilitation from others or himself. Therefore, haw he is functioning at that point as helpee is important, for it is an integral part of the facilitative process. 38 It was decided to use three excerpts for several reasons. Some subjects tended to verbalize the same problem content and affect repeatedly during the screen- ing interview, others had divergent themes of content and affect. Therefore, using three problem statements in- creased the content validity of the instrument. It would appear that this more inclusive sample of problem expres- sion would also increase the face validity of the measure to the subjects. Finally, using the three problem state- ments, rather than just one, should have made for a more reliable instrument (Anastasi, 1968) by providing a larger sample of behavior and thus a more adequate and consistent measure. All excerpting was done by the researcher prior to knowing the group assignment of any subject. Excerpts were chosen only after the entire screening interview was reviewed. One of the three was always taken from the initial expression of concern by the subject. The state— ments ranged from 15 to 130 seconds in length, however, the three statements always totaled a minimum of 150 seconds in length. A posttest tape was made for each subject which COntained his three problem statements and blank segments to record his reSponses. The format for this tape can be Seen in Figure l. 39 . 2—minute 30— "Statement problem number one" / statement / cue / reSponse / cue / second t1me pause Note: Sequence is repeated for all three problem state— ments. FIGURE 1. Pictorial representation of posttest tape for measuring facilitative level of self-help responses The testing format was such that every subject came to a research room in the Counseling Center at an appointed time the week following the sixth treatment session. The testing was administered by the researcher. To reduce sources of bias a standardized set of instruc— tions was shown to every subject (see Appendix 0). If a subject had any questions he was referred back to the in- structions which were known to be adequate. Once a subject indicated he understood the procedure and was ready to begin the researcher left the room and did not return until the test was completed. All mechanical procedures for playing and recording with the test tape were conducted from an adjacent room, therefore, during testing the subject was alone. All of the completed test tapes were recorded on a master tape. The order of subjects on the master tape was randomized. Three independent and experienced raters were given copies of the master tape and asked to rate all helpee responses using the Carkhuff scales for E, R, G, 40 and C. They were provided with a rating form to record their ratings (see Appendix P). Because all three raters were experienced in using the scales they required no special training. From previous experience with the raters the researcher knew them to be functioning above minimally facilitative levels as helpers (above 3.0 on E, R, G, and C). Cannon and Carkhuff (1970) have found rater level of functioning to be an important factor. Essentially, raters functioning below minimally facili- tative levels tend to distort what they hear and thus reduce reliability. Rater reliability was estimated using an analysis of variance method developed by Hoyt (1941). The ANOVA was calculated on the CDC 3600 computer using a program developed by Jennrich (1961). This procedure yielded estimates of reliability of the ratings on the four dependent variables of interest. The results are re- ported in Table 3. TABLE 3 Hoyt Reliability Coefficients for Ratings of Self— Help ReSponses on Facilitative Dimensions Empathy Regard Genuineness Concreteness .957 .918 .901 .936 * 41 Behavior Rating Form The Behavior Rating Form (BRF) was developed by the researcher. It was an l8-item instrument with a 6- level response format. The BRF was administered after treatment to subjects and the significant-other observers whom they identified prior to treatment. The instrument was designed to measure two categories of outcome be- havior: (l) interpersonal functioning, and (2) idio- syncratic behavior changes in the direction of individual subject counseling goals. The response format used for all items on the BRF was as follows: 1. I strongly feel that it is not true. 2. I feel it is not true. 3. I feel it is probably not true, or more untrue than true. 4. I feel it is probably true, or more true than untrue. 5. I feel it is true. 6. I strongly feel it is true. The instructions used for subjects and observers can be Seen in Appendices K and L, respectively. Interpersonal Functioning All subjects had classified their concerns as "personal-social," as distinguished from educational, vocational, or administrative. Such a classification 42 dictates that at least some attention be given to inter- personal relationships. In addition, a rationale for emphasizing a qualitative consideration of interpersonal relationships was presented in Chapter I. There it was posited that dyadic relationships were, to a large degree, facilitating or retarding as a result of the levels of E, R, G, and C within the communication processes. Conse— quently, this instrument sought to evaluate interpersonal functioning on these dimensions. Three items were constructed for each of the four facilitative dimensions. Some of the items were modifi- cations of those in the Relationship Questionnaire develOped by Truax (Truax & Carkhuff, 1967). The 12 items were clustered to measure the single variable of interest—-interpersonal functioning. Subjects' ratings and observers' ratings were considered separately, thus forming two dependent variables for analysis: (1) self ratings on interpersonal functioning, and (2) observers' ratings on interpersonal functioning. Reliability estimates were obtained using an analysis of variance method developed by Hoyt (1941). The ANOVA's were calculated differently for self and observer ratings. The FORTAP program (Baker & Martin, 1970) was used for self ratings, while a program developed by Jennrich (1961) was used for observer ratings because these were average scores (two observers for each subject) 43 and the FORTAP could not handle the non-integers which frequently resulted from averaging. All 12 items with sub-scale and overall reliability coefficients are pre— sented in Table 4. Counseling Goals The second category of outcome behavior which the BRF sought to measure was change on those behaviors identified as directly related to the problems for which subjects were seeking help. Prior to treatment every subject was asked to write down (see Appendix F) at least five such behaviors, the changing of which would consti- tute counseling goals. From these five "goal" behaviors four items were constructed for the BRF. As much as possible the items were framed in the same language used by the subject. However, they were all written in terms of "change" in the direction implied by the subject. The four items were clustered so as to measure a single vari- able of interest--behavior change in the direction of idiosyncratic subject goals. It should be noted at this point that in a real sense the subjects had assistance in identifying some "goal" behaviors. Though the screening counselor did not discuss this.procedure with the subject directly, most screeners were known to function in such a manner so as to help clients clarify some general goals for counseling. Because subjects performed the research task immediately 44 TABLE 4 Interpersonal Items on Behavior Rating Form With Self and Observer Reliability Coefficients Sub-Scale Overall Reliability Reliability Self Observers Self Observers FUNCTIONING INTERPERSONAL EMPATHY Even when others cannot quite say what they mean, he seems to know what they feel. He sometimes understands others so well that he seems to know what they are feeling even when they try to hide their feelings. What he says does not seem to fit what others are feeling. .38 .68 REGARD He communicates a respect for what others have to say, whether he agrees with them or not. He really seems to care about the feelings, experiences, and potentials of others. Others do not feel free to be themselves around him. .45 .65 GENUINENESS He is spontaneous, expressing what he seems to be actually experiencing at the time. He gives genuine feedback, not keeping back his feelings because he thinks it will make others feel one way or another. What he says is different than what he seems to be feeling. .45 .80 CONCRETENESS When discussing a concern with others, he gets down to what really seems to be going on--what the specific issues and feelings are. He is able to help others get the right words for what they are feeling. He does not get down to concrete terms, often remaining general and vague. .66 .78 .79 .88 *Items stated in converse. 45 after their screening interview, there is reason to believe that screening had some effect. However, such an effect was not considered undesirable for it undoubtedly helped some subjects write down more meaningful goals. Also, some counseling researchers consider the setting of goals as an essential and specific counselor-client process (Krumboltz, 1966). Finally, whatever the differential effect between subjects was, it could be considered random between treatment groups. Reliability for self and observer ratings on goals was calculated in the same manner as for interpersonal functioning (see pp. 41—42). The reliability estimates are noted in Table 5. TABLE 5 Self and Observer Reliability Coefficients For Ratings on Idiosyncratic Goal Items* Self Observers .49 .80 *Four items for each subject. A listing of all goal items according to subjects can be found in Appendix M. Thus far, 16 items of the 18-item instrument have been considered--12 interpersonal and 4 goal items. Two 46 additional items were used to provide a check on the appropriateness of the observers. -—"I have had at least as much contact with him over the past three weeks as is usual for us." —-"I consider mySelf in as good a position to make the above responses as most other of his acquaintances." It was planned to disregard any BRF where the observer rated either of these items below level 4 (toward "not true"), but none did so. Before the BRF was ready for administration some final procedures were carried out. First, the 18 items were arranged in random order. Then, several naive indi- viduals were asked to complete the form with a friend or themself in mind. Their suggestions resulted in some minor modifications in wording. Finally, an individual BRF was prepared for each subject, written in the appro~ priate gender and incorporating the idiosyncratic goal items. An example appears in Appendix N. The BRF was administered to subjects and observers the week following the sixth treatment session. It was given to subjects immediately after they had completed the measure on self-help responding, using standardized instructions. Observers were mailed the BRF along with a letter of introduction (see Appendix J), standardized instructions, and a stamped, addressed envelope. The materials used with observers did not identify the subjects as being in counseling, only that they were 47 participating in a social science research project on campus. After repeated follow-up on observers who did not return the BRF after one week, a 95 per cent response rate was realized within two weeks of the sixth treatment session. This return resulted in two observer ratings on all subjects, except for one in each group for whom there was only one rating. Treatment Procedures Experimental Group--Rationale Training in facilitative self—responding incor— porated the (1) modeling, (2) didactic, and (3) experi— ential sources of learning for shaping the facilitative level of trainee responses. Modeling of high-level helper responses was provided by the counselor and the SHeHrS tape. Didactic presentations were made on the facilitative dimensions by the counselor and through the handout en— titled "Basic Dimensions of the Helping Process" (see Appendix I). Also, trainees were instructed in the rationale behind facilitative self-responding and the training process. Experientially, trainees came to know the effect of their counselor's high-level helper re- Sponses and eventually their own. They practiced facili- tative self-responding while receiving feedback and selective reinforcement from their counselor. 48 The necessary skill for discerning a high-level (above 3.0) from a low-level response was known to be a prerequisite for, but not a guarantee of, the skill for initiating a high-level response. Therefore, training was divided into two sequential phases: (1) discrimi- nation, and (2) communication. The overall procedure took place during six one- hour weekly sessions. Counselors were instructed to spend a minimum of two-thirds of that time in direct training activities. The provided guidelines (see Appendix G) summarized the rationale for the study and delineated three sequential stages with suggested time allotments. Experimental Group Format Stage I--Introduction (session 1) 1. Establishing the relationship. Listening to and communicating an understanding of the client's presenting problem was considered an important factor in client motivation, as well as an opportunity for modeling high— level facilitative conditions. 2. Presenting training rationale. The training rationale was summarized for the client so as to optimize his participation and resultant training effectiveness. The counselor pre— sented the rationale from that provided him 49 in the guidelines and framed his presentation in terms of the client's problems as much as possible. Questions concerning participation. The counselor dealt directly with problems related to client participation. These ranged from scheduling difficulties to client motivation. Stage II--Discrimination Training (sessions 1—3) 1. Gross discrimination. Practice in gross rating (good, fair, poor) of helper responses on the SHeHrS tape. Counselor gave feedback, focusing on the dimension of empathy. Presenting the facilitative dimensions (session 2). A didactic presentation was made Of the facilitative dimensions. The client was presented with a copy of the simpli- fied Carkhuff scales for independent study between the second and third sessions. Modeling levels of the facilitative dimensions. The SHeHrS tape was used for modeling high, minimally facilitative, and low levels of E, R, G, and C in self-responding. Rating practice. Clients used the SHeHrS tape to practice rating self—help reSponses on the levels of E, R, G, and C. Counselors gave 50 appropriate feedback—-reinforcing accurate discriminations and correcting faulty ones. Stage III——Communication Training (sessions 4-6) 1. Practicing helper responses. Client responded in the helper role to helpee statements on the SHeHrS tape (statements 14—24) with counselor giving appropriate feedback. Roleplaying helpee and helper roles with counselor. Counselor and client alternated in helpee and helper roles with one another. Client was encouraged to use real problem statements as helpee. During this procedure the counselor focused on the dimensions and role which needed the most improvement. Practice in facilitative self—responding. Client was asked to state a real and personal problem which in turn was recorded. The problem statement was played back and the client was asked to respond as helper, which was also recorded. Then, the entire sequence was played and the helper responses rated by the client on the facilitative dimensions with counselor giving appropriate feedback. Eventually, the procedure was followed without use of the recorder and with increasingly less dependence upon the counselor. 51 Control Group Clients in the control group received individual counseling in one-hour weekly sessions over the same six- week period that the experimental group received indi— vidual training. All counseling sessions were recorded. The counselors were given no special instructions and were not informed about the nature of the treatment in the experimental group. If a client had a question about his participation in the study, he was referred to the re— searcher for the necessary information. Egreatment After Experimental Period All subjects were informed initially that the experimental period was for six weeks only. They were also told that at the end of this period they and their counselor could consider together future treatment pro— cedures. Therefore, all subjects knew of the opportunity for treatment beyond the experimental period and conse- Quently spent some time during the sixth session discuss- ing this option with their counselors. Hypotheses Research or alternate hypotheses will be designated With the letters "HA" numbered 1 through 8. All eight null hypotheses will be formally stated in Chapter III and the SymbOl "H0" will be used to identify them. The first exPeCtation of this study was that counseled clients who 52 received training in facilitative self-responding (Group 1) would respond at higher levels of E, R, G, and C to their own problem statements than would counseled clients who did not receive training (Group 2). Such a prediction implied the examination of four distinct hypotheses: 1. H u > u A1: 1 2 The posttreatment level of empathy in the self—help responses of Group 1 will be higher than the posttreatment level of Group 2. '2. HA2: 111 > 1.12 The posttreatment level of regard in the self—help responses of Group 1 will be higher than the posttreatment level of Group 2. 3. H : u > u A3 1 2 The posttreatment level of genuineness in the self-help responses of Group 1 will be higher than the posttreatment level of Group 2. 4. H : u > u A4 1 2 The posttreatment level of concreteness in the self-help responses of Group 1 will be higher than the posttreatment level of Group 2. A second expectation was that counseled clients Who received training (Group 1) on the facilitative dimensions would function at higher levels interpersonally than would counseled clients who did not receive training (Group 2) as assessed by the clients themselves and SiSnificant-other observers. This prediction implied two additional hypotheses: 53 HA5: 111 > u 2 The posttreatment level of interpersonal functioning of Group 1 will be higher than the posttreatment level of Group 2 as assessed by the subjects. H u > u A6: 1 2 The posttreatment level of interpersonal functioning of Group 1 will be higher than the posttreatment level of Group 2 as assessed by significant-other observers. A third expectation was that counseled clients who received training (Group 1) would show a greater change on behaviors related to pretreatment counseling goals than would counseled clients who did not receive training (Group 2) as assessed by the clients themselves and significant-other observers. Thus, two final hypotheses were implied: 7. HA7: ul > u 2 The posttreatment change on goal behaviors of Group 1 will be greater than the change of Group 2 as assessed by the subjects. H u > u A8: 1 2 The posttreatment change on goal behaviors of Group 1 will be greater than the change of Group 2 as assessed by significant-other observers. 54 Experimental Design This experiment used a factorial elaboration of the "posttest-only control group design" described by Campbell and Stanley (1969). Figure 2 provides a pic— torial representation. Though the statistical tests available for a pretest-posttest design are more powerful, it was decided that pretesting was too great a threat to internal validity, particularly in regard to the effect of pretesting on the posttests and change in test re- spondents over time. Analysis of Data A repeated measures analysis of variance was per— formed on the eight dependent variables. This analysis allowed for considering all eight measures together in a test for treatment main effect, and treatment with mea- Sures interaction. Also, univariate ANOVA's were per- formed to test for a treatment effect on the eight measures individually. The repeated measures ANOVA was calculated k6? a computer program develOped byJennrich (1961), while the univariate ANOVA's were calculated as a part of a Imultivariate program developed by Finn (1970). The re- SUlts will be discussed in Chapter III. M6 M7 M8 5 R < 10 S11 512 T 7- S13 k S20" 55 M1 M2 M3 M4 M5 M6 M7 M8 F 51 S2 S3 T1 . FIGURE 2. Pictorial representation of experimental design Random assignment Subject Experimental treatment group-~individual counseling with training in facilitative self-reSponding Treatment-control group—-individual counseling Empathy level of self-help responses Regard level of self—help responses Genuineness level of self-help responses Concreteness level of self—help responses Level of interpersonal functioning as reported by 8 Level of interpersonal functioning as reported by observers Change on goal behaviors as reported by S Change on goal behaviors as reported by observers CHAPTER III RESULTS Treatment Effects The differential effect of the two treatment pro- cedures on the eight measures can be observed in terms of the resultant means in Table 6. Differences between groups can be compared across measures directly in that the raw scores were transformed so as to standardize the variance across measures and approximate a common metric. The transformation was accomplished by dividing each score on a given measure by the square root of the pooled within experimental and control group variance of that measure. (Raw scores according to subject, counselor, and treatment group appear in Appendix Q.) The means indicate that Group 1 did better than GrCNJp 2 on all measures. The repeated measures analysis Of Variance allowed testing for the overall treatment effect (G), and for treatment by measures interaction (GM). (The main effect for measures (M) was not relevant to the hypotheses of interest.) The results of the analy— Sis are shown in Table 7. 56 57 muw>ummno >9 Oncommmu mamnlmawm .muofi>mnmh Hmom co mmcmnu n m: .HO>OH mmmcmnmuocoo u a: mamm we mmcomwmu mamnlmawm .mHOH>MSOQ Hmom GO wmcmno N NE ~Hw>®H mmmcwcflscmw H m2 mnw>ummno wn mmcommwu mamelwamw .mchOHpOGSM quOmmmmumncH n oz 1Hm>ma pummwm n NE mawm wn wmcommmu mamslmamm .mchOHUOGSM HMCOmuOmnOch u m2 ram>wa mgumafim n as "wmm mmuoom coEuOmmcmuH* mm.b w>.m na.o mm.w on.HH mm.m oa.m wm.m mm.m pcmuw mm.m Hm.v vo.o mo.m om.HH ww.v mm.m oa.m mm.e Houucoo "NU mw.m om.m mm.m mm.m om.mH «H.m am.m mm.> wh.m Hmpcmfiflummxm "H0 386 as E oz m2 :1 m2 N: as mmsouo nemanmwne Houncoo can Hmucmefinmmxm How monummmz ucmpcwdmo so 1.mOHOOm are: m mqmfifi 58 TABLE 7 Summary of the Repeated Measures Analysis of Variance Sum of Mean source Squares df Square F (df) Grand Mean 9225.139 1 9225.139 G 255.228 1 255.228 122.705* (1,18) S:G 37.451 18 2.080 M 541.008 7 77.286 GM 85.579 7 12.225 14.484* (1.18)” SM:G 106.339 126 0.844 Total 1025.598 159 6.450 *Significant beyond the .05 level. **Conservative F, see note. Note: Repeated measures ANOVA assumes that the measures are equally correlated, but there was no basis for making this assumption with the measures used. Therefore, the Geisser-Greenhouse (1958) conservative F test was used which allowed violation of the assumption. With this method the computational procedures for F are identical, but re— duced degrees of freedom are used for determining the criti— cal value of F. The conventional degrees of freedom would have been 7 and 126. 59 The F test statistic for treatment effects was 122.705, which is significant at d = .05. Therefore, the experimental group did significantly better than the con— trol group when considering the eight measures together. Specifically, this implied the rejection of the following null hypothesis: General H0: u1 = u 2 When considering together eight measures of treatment effectiveness, the posttreatment levels of Groups 1 and 2 will not differ from each other. And allowed acceptance of the following alternate hypothesis: General H : A ul>u 2 When considering together eight measures of treatment effectiveness, the posttreatment levels of Group 1 will be greater than Group 2. Interaction Effect The F test statistic for groups with measures inter— action was 14.484, which is significant at d = .05. The Significant interaction effect indicates that the treatment effect was not constant across all measures, in other words, there was an effect due to particular combinations of the leVels of groups and measures. Therefore, the overall treatment effect may not hold for each level of measures. The graph in Figure 3 illustrates that the inter— action was ordinal, Group 1 doing better than Group 2 across all measures. An ordinal interaction such as this is less restrictive to generalization of results than a Means 10 — Group 1 5 Group 2 Measures FIGURE 3. Graph of interaction effect between groups and measures disordinal type. Though measures and groups interact, they do not do so in a contradictory manner. At least two major patterns can be observed from the interaction graph. One is constituted by measures 1 through 4 which represent the facilitative level of self- help reSponses. Clarely, group differences are greater on those dimensions for which Group 1 was directly trained. The second pattern results from measures 5 through 8 which represent the scores on the Behavior Rating Form. For these the group differences are noticeably less, particu— larly for measures 5 and 7, the self—reports. To examine further the group differences on the individual dimensions, or measures, univariate analyses were required. 61 Univariate Effects Univariate analyses of variance allowed for directly testing group differences on each of the depen- dent measures and thus provided tests for the eight specific hypotheses stated in Chapter II. The procedure used was a series of one—way ANOVA's and the results are summarized in Table 8. The univariate results will be considered in accord with the rationale used in constructing the de— pendent measures. Measures 1 through 4 used the ratings of experienced judges to assess the effectiveness of train- ing for particular behaviors--(l) empathic, (2) regardful, (3) genuine, and (4) concrete self—help responses. Mea- sures 5 through 8 were generated by the Behavior Rating Form which focused upon interpersonal functioning as assessed by (5) the subject and his (6) significant— other observers, and change on idiosyncratic behaviors related to counseling goals as assessed by (7) the subject and his (8) significant—other observers. Facilitative Level of Self— Help Responses As shOWn in Table 8, Group 1 did significantly better than Group 2 when self—help responses were rated on each of the facilitative dimensions. These results focus directly on the first four hypotheses of this study: 62 .mmuMHum>Hcd mo menses on» .m an mo. mo mzmam Hamuw>o one mchH>Hp mg pmcHEHmump mma zmocmummmu mo a: maze .mmmoo. v a O>me on can mumaum>flcs one HO>OH mo. map cachmh uCMOHMHcmHm on 09 "muoz .mp0: mom .Hw>ma mo. we» Ocommn “CMOHMHcmHm« .OGGH.4H oaaa.o whom.m mum>nmmno an .muofi>mnmn Hmom co mmcmnu m Gmam.o Gmma.o HmmH.o mamm an .muofi>mcmh Hmom co mmcmnu n «mmma.ma ammm.o nmma.v muw>ummno ha .mchOHuocsm HmcomummnmucH w ooao.m qua.o Gama.o mamm an .qucOHuOcsm HMGOmHmmHmucH m .aakm.mm akoa.o amoa.a mmcoammu armaumamm .HO>OH mmmcwumnocoo v aomma.mo woao.o moma.m mmcommmu damclmamm .HO>OH mmmqmcflscmo m ammvm.Hv mama.o meH.m Oncommmu mamnlmamm .HO>OH pummmm m aamoa.aoa omaa.o onmm.aa Oncommmu mamclmamm .Hm>ma mcnmmEm H Houum cmm3pmm s 0 “ma H .mpv m wumsqm new: mumsqm cams OHQMHHM> mocmflnm> mo mommamca mumflum>flcb mo mumEEdm m 392. 63 Hypothesis 1: H : u 01 < u 1 2 The posttreatment level of empathy in the self-help responses of Group 1 will equal (or be less than) the posttreatment level of Group 2. H01 rejected in favor of HAl' H : > A]. 1.1 u l 2 The posttreatment level of empathy in the self—help responses of Group 1 will be higher than the post- treatment level of Group 2. Hypothesis 2: H02: u1 < u2 The posttreatment level of regard in the self—help responses of Group 1 will equal (or be lower than) the posttreatment level of Group 2. H rejected in favor of H 02 A2' H : u > u A2 1 2 The posttreatment level of regard in the self-help responses of Group 1 will be higher than the posttreatment level of Group 2. Hypothesis 3: H03‘ u1 s u2 The posttreatment level of genuineness in the self— help responses of Group 1 will equal (or be lower than) the posttreatment level of Group 2. H03 rejected in favor of HA3. 64 H : u > u A3 1 2 The posttreatment level of genuineness in the self— help responses of Group 1 will be higher than the posttreatment level of Group 2. Hypothesis 4: H u 04: s u l 2 The posttreatment level of concreteness in the self— help responses of Group 1 will equal (or be lower than) the posttreatment level of Group 2. H04 rejected 1n favor of HA4‘ H : > A4 u u 1 2 The posttreatment level of concreteness in the self— help responses of Group 1 will be higher than the posttreatment level of Group 2. Thus, the statistical significance of training efEEectiveness has been established. However, the issue of "meaaningful significance" remains an important question. hi Chapter I the concept of "minimally facilitative" was discussed and was seen to be quantified as level 3.0 on the 5—point Carkhuff scales. Since the scales were used for measuring the posttreatment self-help responses, the raw score ratings allow direct comparison of Group 1 and Group 2 on the facilitative dimensions as originally de- fined and scaled. If assumed that the control group did not deteriorate in their level, then mean comparisons, as ShOWn in Table 9, indicate that the trained group gained more than one level on each of the dimensions. 65 Such a gain is meaningful by definition of the levels. Also, the trained group achieved essentially minimally facilitative levels on each dimension. The obtained results are evidence that subjects in Group 1 were trained to function as minimally effective self-helpers. TABLE 9 Mean Level of Self-Help Responses on Facilitative Dimensions With Group Differentials (Carkhuff Scales) Group Empathy Regard Genuineness Concreteness l 3.02 2.81 2.99 3.00 2 1.47 1.80 1.88 1.59 Differen- tials 1.55 1.01 1.11 1.41 Interpersonal Functioning Hypotheses five and six focused upon how the treat- ments compared in their effect upon interpersonal function- ing. Hypothesis 5: H ulsu 05‘ 2 The posttreatment level of interpersonal function- ing of Group 1 will equal (or be lower than) the level of Group 2 as reported by the clients. H05 failed to be rejected. 66 Hypothesis 6: H : u 06 s u l 2 The posttreatment level of interpersonal function— ing of Group 1 will equal (or be lower than) the level of Group 2 as reported by significant—other observers. H rejected in favor of H 06 A6' HA6: u1 > u2 The posttreatment level of interpersonal function- ing of Group 1 will be higher than the level of Group 2 as reported by significant-other observers. Change Toward Individual Counseling Goals Hypotheses seven and eight focused upon how treat- ments compared in their effect upon changing behaviors related to idiosyncratic counseling goals. Hypothesis 7: H u < 1.1 07‘ 1‘ 2 The posttreatment change on goal behaviors of Group 1 will be equal (or less than) the change of Group 2 as assessed by the clients. H07 failed to be rejected. Hypothesis 8: H08: u1 S 112 The posttreatment change on goal behaviors of Group 1 will be equal (or less than) the change of Group 2 as assessed by significant—other observers. H08 rejected 1n favor of HA8' 67 H : u > u A8 1 2 The posttreatment change on goal behaviors of Group 1 will be greater than the change of Group 2 as assessed by significant—other observers. The results show that Group 1 did significantly better than Group 2 on both dimensions of the Behavior Rating Form--interpersonal functioning and behavior change toward counseling goals--when assessed by observers, but not when assessed by the subjects. Such results appear to be contradictory in terms of the outcome criteria of pri- mary interest. One plausible explanation for the difference be- tween subject and observer ratings is the differential reliability of the two types of raters. Instrumentation reliability was discussed in Chapter II and the results are summarized in Table 10. TABLE 10 Behavior Rating Form Sub Scale Reliability Coefficients for Subjects and Observers Outcome Dimensions Subjects Observers Interpersonal Functioning .79 .88 Behavior Change Toward Goals .49 .80 The results show observer ratings to be more reliable than subject ratings. As reported in Chapter II, there were two observers for each subject in all but two 68 cases. This larger sample of ratings, together with their higher reliability, lends support for inferring group dif- ferences as indicated by observers. Also, it is to be remembered that though group differences due to subject ratings were not significant, they were in the desired direction. Additional support comes from previous re— search which tends to show counselors and clients to be relatively poor judges of treatment outcomes (Meltzoff & Kornreich, 1970). Such has been the case for judgmental, descriptive, performance, and status type data. Finally, it has been the researcher's observation that clients in general become more critical of their overall behavior repertoire during therapy, the effect of which might well cancel out any real gains in either treatment group as reported on an instrument such as the Behavior Rating Form. Again, the question of meaningful significance is relevant. The meaning of measures 5 through 8 can be further clarified upon examination of the raw score means in the context of the Behavior Rating Form response format from which they were generated. The means appear in Table 11 and their relationship to the response format is illustrated in Figure 4. (The levels of the format are detailed in Chapter II.) It can be observed by inspection of the format that 3.5 is that critical value which differ— entiates whether or not a mean is in the direction of desired behavior. On this basis it can be inferred that 69 TABLE 11 Raw Score Means From Behavior Rating Form Group 1 Group 2 Measures Experimental Control M5 Interpersonal functioning, by self 4.65 4.27 M6 Interpersonal functioning, by observers 4.76 3.45 M7 Change on goal behaviors, by self 4.40 4.23 M8 Change on goal behaviors, by observers 4.26 3.17 6 A H 5 E2: Group 1 r——~—""_*“-al\\‘_____‘__—_* -Higg mr:u Group 2 4 O ®4J v.9: U H: m o m n-a s -r-4:>H wrap 2 6.: U m.p n o p 1 ix ' f l I I M5 M6 M7 M8 FIGURE 4. Illustration of Behavior Rating Form response format in relation to group means on measures 5 through 8 70 the statistically significant differences between groups on measures 6 and 8 are also meaningful differences. That is, observers reported only Group 1 to be functioning inter— personally in a positive manner, and behaving in accord with individual counseling goals. Applying the same cri— teria to measures 5 and 7 it is apparent that groups were not different in terms of meaning or statistical analysis. Subject Mortalipy Of the 22 subjects initially participating in this study, 1 terminated prematurely from each treatment group. In Group 1, a female subject was "no show" for the first session and only came three times during the six-week experimental period. Her counselor's assessment was that she was not motivated for any therapeutic intervention at that time. In Group 2, a female terminated after the first three sessions, stating that she had received all the help she wanted at that time. Her counselor also attributed her early termination to insufficient motivation. The final distribution of subjects and counselors within groups appears in Table 12. 71 TABLE 12 Final Subject and Counselor Distribution Within Treatment Groups Sex of Subjects Ccyunselors Number of Subjects Females Males Experimental 1 5 3 2 2 2 1 l 3 1 0* 1 4 _2 1 1 Totals 10 5 5 Control 5 3 l 2 6 2 2 0 7 2 2 0 8 2 l 1 9 _1 1* 1 Totals 10 6 4 *Subject mortality 2&1. 2X22. E 1X3 m 114° A5: A6: A7: A8: 72 Status of Research Hypotheses The posttreatment level of empathy in the self-help responses of Group 1 will be higher than the post— treatment level of Group 2. Confirmed. The posttreatment level of regard in the self-help responses of Group 1 will be higher than the post- treatment level of Group 2. Confirmed. The posttreatment level of genuineness in the self- help responses of Group 1 will be higher than the posttreatment level of Group 2. Confirmed. The posttreatment level of concreteness in the self- help responses of Group 1 will be higher than the posttreatment level of Group 2. Confirmed. The posttreatment level of interpersonal functioning of Group 1 will be higher than the posttreatment level of Group 2 as assessed by the subjects. Not Confirmed. ——_ The posttreatment level of interpersonal functioning of Group 1 will be higher than the posttreatment level of Group 2 as assessed by significant-other observers. Confirmed. The posttreatment change on goal behaviors of Group 1 will be greater than the change of Group 2 as assessed by the subjects. Not Confirmed. The posttreatment change on goal behaviors of Group 1 will be greater than the change of Group 2 as assessed by significant-other observers. Confirmed. CHAPTER IV DISCUSSION Overview This experiment examined two fundamental questions: (1.) Can clients be trained to respond to their own problems at: minimally facilitative levels of empathy (E), regard (Ii), genuineness (G), and concreteness (C)? And, (2) does Sllch training in facilitative self-responding have a posi— ‘tjsve effect upon dimensions of client benefit, specifically, iIlterpersonal functioning, and behavior change in the (iirrection of individual counseling goals? To seek answers for these questions two treatment Clcnnditions were established with the intent of making Sfifstematic comparisons of outcome. Twenty-two subjects VVEEre randomly assigned to one of two treatment conditions, (Dr‘ groups. The subjects were college students who were Seneking individual counseling for personal-social concerns and who had volunteered to participate. Subjects in Group 1 received individual counseling integrated with individual systematic training in facilitative self— responding. Subjects in Group 2 received individual 73 74 counseling with no systematic training. Both groups received treatment for one hour a week over a six-week period. Four counselors provided the treatment for Group 1, while five different counselors provided treatment for Group 2. All nine counselors were judged equivalent on the dimensions of helper functioning using the Carkhuff scales for E, R, G, and C. They were also deemed equiva- lent on the bases of experience, level of training, sex, age, and general therapeutic orientation. The procedure used for training in facilitative self—responding was developed from previous work that Cark— liuff and associates had done in regard to facilitative interpersonal functioning. Hence, the procedure was an extension of the Carkhuff model and employed modeling, didactic, and experiential sources of learning to shape 1ligher levels of E, R, G, and C in self-help responses. Training followed two sequential phases: (1) training in tile discrimination of levels on each of the dimensions, arni (2) training in the communication of increasingly Iligher levels of each of the dimensions in response to the trainee's problems. To directly measure posttreatment training effec— tiveness subjects in Groups 1 and 2 were cast into the self—help role by being asked to respond as helper to three problem statements which were taken from their own 75 intake interview recording. The responses were subse- quently rated by three independent judges on each of the facilitative dimensions. The results showed Group 1 to be functioning at about the 3.0 level on all dimensions and at about one level higher than Group 2. Univariate analyses of variance indicated the differences were sta- tistically significant (d = .05). The Behavior Rating Form (BRF) was developed to measure group differences beyond the behaviors for which Group 1 was directly trained and behaviors more obviously related to client benefit. Specifically, the BRF measured interpersonal functioning with items based upon the con— structs of E, R, G, and C. Also, it measured behavior change in the direction of individual counseling goals with items based upon idiosyncratic problem behaviors identified by each subject prior to treatment. The BRF was administered to each subject and the two significant other observers whom each had identified prior to treat— ment. Univariate analyses of variance showed Group 1 to be significantly better (a = .05) than Group 2 on the dimensions of interpersonal functioning and behavior change toward counseling goals as reported by observers, but not as reported by subjects. The differences reported by observers were not only significant but were meaningful. That is, according to observers the behavior of Group 1 was in the desired direction and that of Group 2 was not. 76 In addition to the univariate analyses above, overall treatment differences were assessed by considering all eight dependent measures together in a repeated measures analysis of variance. The results showed that Group 1 did significantly (d = .05) better than Group 2, but also yielded a significant ordinal interaction for groups and measures . Limitations Before considering the many positive conclusions ’ and implications of the present study, it is appropriate to review some of its limitations so as to properly qualify inferences and to suggest directions for future research. In terms of the internal validity of the experi— Inental design, the "posttest only control group design" UCampbell & Stanley, 1969) controls well for variables thich might confound the experimental treatment effect. Phswever, because counselors were not randomly assigned tr) treatment conditions there remains this possible source qpect a subject who had been trained to function at hixgher levels with himself to do so with others, parti— CUJJiIly since training methodology incorporated many intxarpersonal elements (see Chapter II). But testing this exPeectation was not the only basis for selecting the inter— Personal criterion. The value of empathic, regardful, genuddne, and concrete interpersonal communication was elabcnaated upon in Chapter I, where these dimensions were ShOWTl'tO account substantially for the quality of 84 interpersonal relationships and subsequent psychological well—being. The results showed significant and meaningful differences on the interpersonal dimension for observer ratings, but not for subject ratings. Some plausible explanations for the different results were considered in Chapter III, including the lower reliability for subject ratings, fewer subject than observer ratings, and previous research showing subjects to be relatively poor evaluators of their own therapy. Another consideration is the relative homogeneity of the subject population when compared to the observer population. Clearly, there were several systematic selection factors which served to re— duce differences between subjects which were not operating for observers. Such homogeneity would seem to call for a more highly reliable measure than the BRF to discriminate even meaningful differences. In this context it is important to recall that the non-significant differences for subject ratings were in the direction favoring facili- tative self-responding. Though there was not statistical support from subject ratings favoring the experimental procedure, neither was there statistical evidence directly contradicting the differences reported by observers. Finally, there is a logical basis for giving greater validity to observer ratings. Observers would be rela— tively free from confounding experimental effects, such 85 as loyalty to counselor, and generally more objective than subjects making self-ratings. And two observers, probably functioning independently, would seem to be better judges of the interpersonal conditions being offered them than would the single subject doing the offering. The second dimension related to direct client benefit was change on those behaviors which the individual subjects identified as associated with the problems for which they were seeking help. These behavioral correlates of the presenting personal-social concern of the client appeared to be fundamentally appropriate outcome criteria. The dimension rests upon the assumption that individuals seek counseling because they want something to change-- some personally meaningful difference in their behavior. At times there is a tendency in the behavioral sciences to consider variables of particular interest only to the researcher, perhaps far removed from the "real world" of the client. The inclusion of the goal dimension in this study was an attempt to assure client relevancy in the outcome criteria and subsequent generalizations of the results. Results on the goal dimension were parallel to those for interpersonal functioning in that there were significant and meaningful group differences for observer ratings, but not for subject ratings. The possible expla— nations sighted above for observer and subject differences 86 are also applicable for goals. However, it should be noted that the reliability for subject ratings on goals was low- est of all outcome measures--.49. Hence, the group differ- ences would have had to been relatively large for statisti- cal significance on this measure. Also, it should be recalled that there were only 4 goal items on the BRF as compared to 12 interpersonal items. The 4 items reflected a wide range of goals, yet their scores were added to comprise a single measure of "change toward goals." This adding procedure gave the best single estimate of the parameter of interest, but necessarily ignored such factors as differential goal priorities and categorical differences of the behaviors involved. It is likely that facilitative self-responding is more effective for some types of prob- lems than others, but this issue was left to further research. Overall, the evidence shows that facilitative self—responding is a viable therapeutic intervention. In terms of the two fundamental questions examined, the answers, though tentative, are affirmative--clients can be so trained, and it does make a meaningful difference. A short-term helping program such as this has numerous implications, many of which have been noted pre- viously. However, some additional broad implications are worth consideration. The Carkhuff training model for facilitative interpersonal functioning has been successfully used in 87 groups. Though additional research is needed, there is reason to expect that the extension of the Carkhuff model employed in this study can also be used successfully in the group context. Group training would probably be more efficient in terms of time and manpower, but there is also promise for an integration with the interpersonal realm, thus providing a truly comprehensive training program. One advantage to an integrated program would be better representation of "real world" interpersonal situations. When a problem developed within the life of the group, the individuals involved could move into the most direct form of experiential training. Another kind of integration is implied upon con- sidering the potential of facilitative self—responding as a therapeutic adjunct. Undoubtedly some clients function in such a manner that this treatment would be of limited help by itself. However, this does not rule out its adjunctive application. Facilitative self-responding would seem particularly complementary to the "insight therapies" which have often been criticized for failing to aid clients in learning the skills necessary to main- tain their gains and improve functioning independently. Also, its use as a pre-therapy training seems worth further exploration. Every experienced therapist knows about the so-called "good client"--usually referring to the client who deals with problem content and affect in such a manner 88 that he "moves" in therapy. Somehow this client knows how to use the helping relationship effectively, and engages in high levels of self-eXploration with positive results. The skills taught in facilitative self-responding appear closely related to what characterizes the "good client." Thus, it might well serve as a method of training clients to be "good clients" and consequently optimize the effi— ciency and effectiveness of therapy. Such an application seems particularly relevant for counseling agencies with long waiting lists for treatment. Finally, by its essential nature facilitative self-responding is educational as well as therapeutic. The learning and relearning involved would seem applicable to a broader population than self—referred counseling clientele. Just as formal educational systems are moving toward greater incorporation of training in interpersonal skills, so might training in self—help behavior be in— corporated. Increasing numbers of educators and students --he1pers and helpees--are calling for greater emphasis on the skills and conditions necessary to live effectively in a rapidly changing and complex world. Facilitative self- responding is a promising approach toward that goal of "effective living." Effectiveness not only defined in terms of research instruments, but also by the sense of well-being that comes with having adequate personal re— sources for confronting the problems in living. LIST OF REFERENCES LI ST OF REFERENCES Anastasi, A. Psychological testing. New York: Mac— millan Company, 1968. Baker, F. B., & Martin, T. J. Fortap: A Fortran test analysis package. Occasional Paper No. 10, 1970, Office of Research Consultation, College of Edu- cation, Michigan State University. I Barrett-Lennard, G. T. Dimensions of therapist response as causal factors in therapeutic change. Psychologi— cal Monographs, 1962, Z§(43), Whole No. 562. Barron, F., & Leary, T. Changes in psychoneurotic patients with and without psychotherapy. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 1955, 19, 239-245. Berenson, D. The effects of teacher training in inter- personal skills upon student performance. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Fordham University, 1970. Berenson, B. G., Carkhuff, R. R., & Myrus, P. The inter— personal functioning and training of college students. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 1966, 13, 441-446. Berenson, B. G., Friel, T., & Mitchell, K. L. Factor analysis of therapeutic conditions for high— and low— functioning therapists. Journal of Clinical Psychology, in press, 1970. Berenson, B. G., & Mitchell, K. L. Confrontation in counseling and life. Mimeographed manuscript, American International College, Springfield, Mass., 1969. Bierman, R., Carkhuff, R. R., & Santilli, M. Efficacy of empathic communication groups for inter-city pre— school teachers and family workers. Journal of Applied Behavioral Sciences, in press, 1971. Campbell, D. T., & Stanley, J. C. Experimgptal and quasi— experimental designs for research. Chicago: Rand McNally & Company, 1967. 89 90 Cannon, J. C., & Carkhuff, R. R. The effect of rater level of functioning and experience upon the discrimination of facilitative conditions. Journal of Consulting Psychology, in press, 1970. Cannon, J. R., & Pierce, R. Order effects in the experi— mental manipulation of therapeutic conditions. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 1968, 24, 242—244. Carkhuff, R. R. Helping and human relations: A primer for lay and professional helpers. Vol. 1. Selection and trainin . New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston, 1969. (a) Carkhuff, R. R. Helping and human relations: A primer for lay and professional helpers. Vol. 2. Practice and research. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston, 1969. b Carkhuff, R. R. Helper communication and discrimination as a function of the helpee affect and content. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 1969, l§(2), 126- 131. (c) Carkhuff, R. R. The development of human resources: Education and social action. New York: Holt, Rine- hart & Winston, 1971. (a) Carkhuff, R. R. Training as a preferred mode of treatment. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 1971, 18(2), 123-131. (b) Carkhuff, R. R., Banks, G., Berenson, B. G., Griffin, A. H., & Hall, R. The selection and training of correctional counselors. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 1971, in press. Carkhuff, R. R., & Berenson, B. G. Beyond counseling and therapy. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston, 1967. Carkhuff, R. R., & Bierman, R. Training as a preferred mode of treatment of parents of emotionally disturbed children. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 1970, 11(2), 157-161. Carkhuff, R. R., & Griffin, A. H. The selection and train- ing of functional professionals for the inner—city pre-school. Journal of Research and Development in Education, 1971, in press. 91 Carkhuff, R. R., Kratochvil, D., & Friel, T. The effects of professional training: The communication and discrimination of facilitative conditions. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 1968, 22(1), 68-74. Carkhuff, R. R., & Truax, C. B. Training in counseling and psychotherapy: An evaluation of an integrated didactic and experiental approach. Journal of Con— sulting Psychology, 1965, 22, 333—336. (a) Carkhuff, R. R., & Truax, C. B. Lay mental health counseling: The effectiveness of lay group counsel- ing. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 1965, 22, 426—431. (b) Cartwright, R. D., & Vogel, J. L. A comparison of changes in psychoneurotic patients during matched periods of therapy and no therapy. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 1960, 22, 121-127. Eysenck, H. J. The effects of psychotherapy: An evalu- ation. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 1952, $9! 319-324. Finn, J. D. Univariate and multivariate analysis of variance and covariance: A Fortran IV program. Occasional Paper No. 9, 1970, Office of Research Consultation, College of Education, Michigan State University. Geisser, S., & Greenhouse, S. W. An extension of Box's results on the use of the F distribution in multi— variate analysis. Annals of Mathematical Statistics, 1958, 22, 885—981. Halkides, G. An investigation of therapeutic success as a function of four variables. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Chicago, 1958. Hoyt, C. J. Test reliability estimated by analysis of variance. Psychometrika, 1941, p, 153-160. Jennrich, R. I. 1604 analysis of variance. Laborator Bulletin, 1961, Numerical Anslysis Department, Un1— vers1ty of Wisconsin. Kagan, N., Krathwohl, D. R., Goldberg, A. D., Campbell, R. J., Schauble, P. G., Greenberg, S. B., Danish, S. J., Resnikoff, A., Bowes, J., & Bondy, S. B. Studies in human interaction: Interpersonal process recall stimulated by videotape. Educational Publi— cation Services, Michigan State University, 1967. 92 Kagan, N., Schauble, P., Resnikoff, A., Danish, 8., & Krathwohl, D. Interpersonal process recall. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 1969, 148, 365-374. Kratochvil, D. Changes in values and interpersonal functioning of nurses in training. Counselor Education and Supervision, 1969, 2, 104-107. Krumboltz, J. D. Behavioral goals for counseling. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 1966, 22(2), 153-159. Krumboltz, J. D. Future directions for counseling research. In J. M. Whiteley (Ed.) Research in counseling. Colum- bus: Charles E. Merrill Publishing Company, 1967. Pp. 184-203. Meltzoff, J., & Kornreich, M. Research in psychotherapy. New York: Atherton Press, Inc., 1970. ‘ Mink, O. G., & Isaksen, H. L. A comparison of effective— ness of nondirective therapy and clinical counseling in the junior high school. School Counselor, 1959, 6, 12-14. Muehlberg, N., Drasgow, J., & Pierce, R. The factorial dimensions of high, moderate, and low functioning therapists. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 1969, 25, 93-95. Pagell, W., Carkhuff, R. R., & Berenson, B. G. The pre— dicted differential effects of the level of counselor functioning upon the level of functioning of out- patients. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 1967, 22, 510-512. Pierce, R., & Drasgow, J. Teaching facilitative inter- personal functioning to psychiatric patients. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 1969, 2Q, 195—198. Pierce, R., & Schauble, P. G. Graduate training of facili— tative counselors: The effects of individual super- vision. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 1970, 22(3), 210—215. Rogers, C. R. On becoming a person. Boston: Houghton Mifflin Company, 1961. Rogers, C. R. The necessary and sufficient conditions of therapeutic personality change. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 1957, 22, 95-103. 93 Rogers, C. R., Gendlin, E., Kiesler, D., & Truax, C. B. (Eds.). The therapeutic relationship and its impact: A study of psychotherapy with schizophrenics. Madison: University of Wisconsin Press, 1967. Scharf, K. R. Training of resident assistants and peer group members in the communication interactional pro— cess skills of empathic understanding of student feeling and student depth of self-exploration. Un- published doctoral dissertation, Michigan State Uni- versity, 1971. Schauble, P. G., & Pierce, R. Owning of feeling in inter- personal process: A scale of measurement. Unpublished manuscript, Michigan State University, 1970. Truax, C. B. The process of group psychotherapy: Relation— ships between hypothesized therapeutic conditions and intrapersonal exploration. Psychological Monographs, 1961, 22(7), Whole No. 511. Truax, C. B., & Carkhuff, R. R. Toward effective counsel— ing and psychotherapy. Chicago: Aldine Publishing Company, 1967. Truax, C. B., Carkhuff, R. R., & Kodman, F. The relation— ships between therapist-offered conditions and patient change in group psychotherapy. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 1965, 22, 327-329. Vitalo, R. The effects of training in interpersonal functioning upon psychiatric inpatients. In R. R. Carkhuff, Helping and human relations. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston, 1969. APPENDICES APPENDIX A CARKHUFF SCALES ON FACILITATIVE DIMENSIONS Scale 1 Empathic Understanding in Interpersonal Processes. II A Scale for Measurement1 Robert R. Carkhuff Level 1 The verbal and behavioral expressions of the first person either do not attend to or detract significant2y from the verbal and behavioral expressions of the second person(s) in that they communicate significantly less of the second person's feelings than the second person has communicated himself. Examples: The first person communicates no awareness of even the most obvious, expressed surface feelings of the second person. The first person may be bored or disinterested or simply operating from a preconceived frame of reference which totally excludes that of the other person(s). In summary, the first person does everything but express that he is listening, understanding or being sensitive to even the feelings of the other person in such a way as to detract significantly from the communications of the second person. Level 2 While the first person responds to the expressed feelings of the second person(s), he does so in such a way that he subtracts noticeable affect from the communications of the ' second person. Examples: The first person may communicate some awareness of obvious surface feelings of the second person but his communications drain off a level of the affect and distort the level of meaning. The first person may communicate his own ideas of what may be going on but these are not congruent with the expressions of the second person. In Summary, the first person tends to respond to other than what the second person is expressing or indicating. 94 95 Scale 1 - p. 2 (Rev. 7/67b) Level 3 The expressions of the first person in response to the ex— pressed feelings of the second person(s) are essentially interchangeable with those of the second person in that they express essentially the same affect and meaning. Example: The first person responds with accurate under- standing of the surface feelings of the second person but may not respond to or may misinterpret the deeper feelings. In summary, the first person is responding so as to neither subtract from nor add to the expressions of the second per- son; but he does not respond accurately to how that person really feels beneath the surface feelings. Level 3 con— stitutes the minimal level of facilitative interpersonal functioning. Level 4 The responses of the first person add noticeably to the ex- pressions of the second person(s) in such a way as to ex- press feelings a level deeper than the second person was able to express himself. Example: The facilitator communicates his understanding of the expressions of the second person at a level deeper than they were expressed, and thus enables the second person to experience and/or express feelings which he was unable to express previously In summary, the facilitator's responses add deeper feeling and meaning to the expressions of the second person. 1The present scale "Empathic understanding in interpersonal processes" has been derived in part from "A Scale for the measurement of accurate empathy" by C. B. Truax which has been validated in extensive process and outcome research on counseling and psychotherapy (summarized in Truax and Carkhuff, 1967) and in part from an earlier version which has been validated in extensive process and outcome research on counseling and psychotherapy (summarized 1n Carkhuff and Berenson, 1967). In addition, similar measures of similar constructs have received extensive sup- POrt in the literature of counseling and therapy and edu— cation. The present scale was written to apply to all interpersonal processes and represent a systematic attempt to reduce the ambiguity and increase the reliability of the Scale. In the process many important delineations and 96 Scale 1 — p. 3 (Rev. 7/67b) Level 5 The first person's responses add s2gnificantly to the feel— ing and meaning of the expressions of the second person(s) in such a way as to (l) accurately express feelings levels below what the person himself was able to express or (2) in the event of ongoing deep self—exploration on the second person's part to be fully with him in his deepest moments. Examples: The facilitator responds with accuracy to all of the person's deeper as well as surface feelings. He is "together" with the second person or "tuned in" on his wavelength. The facilitator and the other person might proceed together to explore previously unexplored areas of human existence. In summary, the facilitator is responding with a full awareness of who the other person is and a comprehensive and accurate empathic understanding of his most deep feelings. additions have been made, including in particular the change to a systematic focus upon the additive, subtractive or interchangeable aspects of the levels of communication of understanding, For comparative purposes: Level 1 of the present scale is approximately equal to Stage 1 of the Truax scale. The remaining levels are approximately cor- respondent: Level 2 and Stages 2 and 3 of the earlier version; Level 3 and Stages 4 and 5; Level 4 and Stages 6 and 77 Level 5 and Stages 8 and 9. The levels of the present scale are approximately equal to the levels of the earlier version of this scale. 97 Scale 2 The Communication of Respect in Inter- personal Processes. II A Scale for Measurement1 Robert R. Carkhuff Level 1 The verbal and behavioral expressions of the first person communicate a clear lack of respect (or negative regard) for the second person(s). Example: The first person communicates to the second per- son that the second person's feelings and exper- iences are not worthy of consideration or that the second person is not capable of acting con- structively. The first person may become the sole focus of evaluation. In summary, in many ways the first person communicates a total lack of respect for the feelings, experiences and potentials of the second person. Level 2 The first person responds to the second person in such a way as to communicate little respect for the feelings and ex- periences and potentials of the second person. Example: The first person may respond mechanically or passively or ignore many of the feelings of the second person. In summary, in many ways the first person displays a lack of respect or concern for the second person's feelings, experiences and potentials. Level 3 The first person communicates a positive respect and con- cern for the second person's feelings, experiences and potentials. Example: The first person communicates respect and concern for the second person's ability to express him- self and to deal constructively with his life and situation. In summary, in many ways the first person communicates that who the second person is and what he does matters to the first person. Level 3 constitutes the minimal level of facilitative interpersonal functioning. 98 Scale 2 - p. 2 (Rev. 7/67b) Level 4 The facilitator clearly communicates a very deep respect and concern for the second person. Example: The facilitator's responses enables the second person to feel free to be himself and to experi— ence being valued as an individual. In summary, the facilitator communicates a very deep caring for the feelings, experiences and potentials of the second person. Level 5 The facilitator communicates the very deepest respect for the second person's worth as a person and his potentials as a free individual. Example: The facilitator cares very deeply for the human potentials of the second person. In summary, the facilitator is committed to the value of the other person as a human being. 1The present scale, "Respect or Positive Regard in Inter—personal Processes," has been derived in part from "A tentative scale for the measurement of unconditional posi— tive regard" by C. B. Truax which has been validated in extensive process and outcome research on counseling and psychotherapy (summarized in Truax and Carkhuff, 1967) and in part from an earlier version which has been validated in extensive process and outcome research on counseling and psychotherapy (summarized in Carkhuff and Berenson, 1967). In addition, similar measures of similar constructs have received extensive support in the literature of counseling and therapy and education. The present scale was written to apply to all interpersonal processes and represents a systematic attempt to reduce the ambiguity and increase the reliability of the scale. In the process many important delineations and additions have been made. For comparative purposes, the levels of the present scale are approximately equal to the stages of both the earlier scales, although the systematic emphasis upon the positive regard rather than upon unconditionality represents a pronounced diver- gence of emphasis and the systematic deemphasis of concern for advice-giving and directionality, both of which may or may not communicate high levels as well as low levels of reSpect. 99 Scale 3 Facilitative Genuineness in Interpersonal Processes A Scale for Measurement1 Robert R. Carkhuff Level 1 The first person's verbalizations are clearly unrelated to what he is feeling at the moment, or his only genuine re— sponses are negative in regard to the second person(s) and appear to have a totally destructive effect upon the second person. Example: The first person may be defensive in her inter- action with the second person(s) and this de- fensiveness may be demonstrated in the content of his words or his voice quality and where he is defensive he does not employ his reaction as a basis for potentially valuable inquiry into the relationship. In summary, there is evidence of a considerable discrepancy between the first person's inner experiencing and his cur— rent verbalizations or where there is no discrepancy, the first person's reactions are employed solely in a destruc— tive fashion. Level 2 The first person's verbalizations are slightly unrelated to what he is feeling at the moment or when his responses are genuine they are negative in regard to the second person and the first person does not appear to know how to employ his negative reactions constructively as a basis for inquiry into the relationship. Example: The first person may respond to the second per— son(s) in a "professional" manner that has a re— hearsed quality or a quality concerning the way helper "should" respond in that situation. In summary, the first person is usually responding according to his prescribed "role" rather than to express what he personally feels or means and when he is genuine his re— sponses are negative and he is unable to employ them as a basis for further inquiry. 100 Scale 3 — p. 2 Level 3 The first person provides no "negative" cues between what he says and what he feels, but he provides no positive cues to indicate a really genuine response to the second person(s). Example: The first person may listen and follow the second person(s) but commits nothing more of himself. In summary, the first person appears to make appropriate responses which do not seem insincere but which do not re- flect any real involvement either. Level 3 constitutes the minimal level of facilitative interpersonal functioning. Level 4 The facilitator presents some positive cues indicating a genuine response (whether positive or negative) in a non— destructive manner to the second person(s). Example: The facilitator's expressions are congruent with his feelings although he may be somewhat hesitant about expressing them fully. In summary, the facilitator responds with many of his own feelings and there is no doubt as to whether he really means what he says and he is able to employ his responses what— ever the emotional content, as a basis for further inquiry into the relationship. Level 5 The facilitator is freely and deeply himself in a non— exploitative relationship with the second person(s). 1The present scale, "Facilitative genuineness in interpersonal processes" has been derived in part from "A tentative scale for the measurement of therapist genuineness or self-congruence" by C. B. Truax which has been validated in extensive process and outcome research on counseling and psychotherapy (summarized in Truax and Carkhuff, 1967) and in part from an earlier version which has been similarly validated (summarized in Carkhuff and Berenson, 1967). In addition, similar measures of similar constructs have re- ceived support in the literature of counseling and therapy and education. The present scale was written to apply to all interpersonal processes and represents a systematic attempt to reduce the ambiguity and increase the reliability 101 Scale 3 - p. 3 Example: The facilitator is completely spontaneous in his interaction and open to experiences of all types, both pleasant and hurtful; and in the event of hurtful responses the facilitator's comments are employed constructively to open a further area of inquiry for both the facilitator and the second person. In summary, the facilitator is clearly being himself and yet employing his own genuine responses constructively. of the scale. In the process, many important delineations and additions have been made. For comparative purposes, the levels of the present scale are approximately equal to the stages of the earlier scale, although the systematic emphasis upon the constructive employment of negative re- aCtions represent a pronounced divergence of emphasis. 102 Scale 5 Personal2y Relevant Concreteness or Specificity of Expression A Scale for Measurement1 Robert R. Carkhuff Level 1 The first person leads or allows all discussion with the second person(s) to deal only with vague and anonymous generalities. Example: The first person and the second person discuss everything on strictly an abstract and highly intellectual level. In summary, the first person makes no attempt to lead the discussion into the realm of personally relevant specific situations and feelings. Level 2 The first person frequently leads or allows even discussions of material personally relevant to the second person(s) to be dealt with on a vague and abstract level. Example: The first person and the second person may dis— cuss "real" feelings but they do so at an ab— stract,intellectualized level. In summary, the first person does not elicit discussion of most personally relevant feelings and experiences in speci- fic and concrete terms. Level 3 The first person at times enables the second person(s) to discuss personally relevant material in specific and con- crete terminology. Example: The first person will help to make it possible for the discussion with the second person(s) to center directly around most things which are per— sonally important to the second person(s) al- though there will continue to be areas not dealt with concretely and areas which the second person does not develop fully in specificity. In summary, the first person sometimes guides discussions into consideration of personally relevant specific and con- crete instances, but these are not always fully developed. Level 3 constitutes the minimal level of facilitative functioning. 103 Scale 5 - p. 2 Level 4 The facilitator is frequently helpful in enabling the second person(s) to fully develop in concrete and specific terms almost all instances of concern. Example: The facilitator is able on many occasions to guide the discussion to specific feelings and experi- ences of personally meaningful material. In summary, the facilitator is very helpful in enabling the discussion to center around specific and concrete instances of most important and personally relevant feelings and experiences. Level 5 The facilitator is always helpful in guiding the discussion so that the second person(s) may discuss fluently, directly, and completely specific feelings and experiences. Example: The first person involves the second person in a discussion of specific feelings, situations, and events, regardless of their emotional content. In summary, the facilitator facilitates a direct expression of all personally relevant feelings and experiences in concrete and specific terms. 1The present scale "Personally Relevant Concreteness or Specificity of Expression" has been derived from earlier work by C. B. Truax (summarized in Truax and Carkhuff, 1967L Similar measures of similar constructs have been researched only minimally. The present scale has received support in research on training and counseling (summarized in Carkhuff and Berenson, 1967). The systematic emphasis upon the personally meaningful relevance of concrete and specific expressions represents a pronounced divergence of emphasis. APPENDIX B LETTER TO SCREENING COUNSELORS November 30, 1970 TO: FROM: Dave Cabush SUBJ: Screening -— Cooperation with a research project During the first two weeks of Winter Term, January 4 - 22, I need to obtain subjects for my thesis research. Volunteers will be sought from those students going through screening during that period, therefore, I am seeking the cooperation of several screeners. Client participation will be subject to final app approval by the Screening Committee. Those participating will receive individual counseling from male interns for as long as appropriate, though the research period will be for only six weeks, January 25 — March 5. They will be randomly assigned to either "traditional" counseling, or an "inno— vative" approach where the traditional relationship will be integrated with training the client to function more ef- fectively as his own helper (employing an extension of the Carkhuff model for facilitative interpersonal functioning). Dr. Cecil Williams is my research sponsor and I have discussed the project with Drs. Mueller and Abeles according to their respective functions. If you are willing to assist me in obtaining my sample over the two week period, please follow the pro- cedures as outlined on the attached page. 104 APPENDIX C PROCEDURAL GUIDELINES FOR SCREENING COUNSELORS OBTAINING SAMPLE--PROCEDURES FOR SCREENING COUNSELOR Record screening interviews a. I will furnish tapes and recorder. b. Obtain "Problem Statement"—-This is the primary pur— pose for recording the interview. What I am seeking is how the client is best able to state (uninter- rupted) the problem(s) for which he is seeking c counseling. You may find it helpful to structure this by at some point making a request, like . . . "As best you can, state the problem for which you are seeking counseling." Subject characteristics a. Male or female b. Sophomore or junior at M.S.U. 0. Problem is primarily personal—social (PS) in nature d. Problem is not urgent or of a crisis nature e. There is not a substantial history of psychopathol— ogy, e.g., psychiatric hospitalization, previous long—term professional psychological help, etc. f. Client appears willing to make at least a 6—week commitment to counseling. Obtaining client's consent a. Ask the client if he would be willing to participate by simply reviewing with him the information on the cover letter and Consent Form. Clients agreeing to participate, and approved by the Screening Com- mittee, can be assured of starting counseling the week of January 25th. (This may be an incentive for some in light of the waiting list.) b. Have client sign the Consent Form. Escort the client to the Test Office where he should complete Part I before leaving the Center. This will take about 15 minutes to complete. Place the tape and Consent Form in my box for those clients agreeing to participate. Make the notation "SUBJECT" at the top of the screening notes. THANK YOU! 105 APPENDIX D STANDARD LETTER OF EXPLANATION TO PROSPECTIVE SUBJECTS MICHIGAN STATE UNIVERSITY East Lansing, Michigan 48823 Counseling Center Student Services Building January 4, 1971 Dear Student: We at the Counseling Center believe that one of the ways we have of becoming increasingly helpful to students like your- self is through careful study of the varying approaches to individual counseling. Therefore, we are asking that you help us in our study of counseling approaches and their effectiveness by participating in a research project that we are currently undertaking. As a participant you will be assigned to a counselor whose primary concern is helping you. You may find his approach in some ways different from what you expected, but he will be using proven concepts and methods, and there will be nothing secretive or mysterious about the process. Again, his primary concern is to be of help to you! If you agree to participate, you are asked to complete the attached form today. It is entitled PART I and has two sections. In Section A you are to list as many different ways as possible that your problem(s) affects your behavior, or put another way, what would you hope to be doing differ- ently as a result of resolving your problem? In Section B you are asked for the names and addresses of two individuals who know you and are in a position to observe your behavior during Winter Term. After you have had about six sessions with your counselor the two individuals you named will be asked to respond to a brief checklist on which they will indicate behavior changes they have observed in recent weeks. The research period will be for six weeks, January 25 - March 5. During the week of March 8th you will be asked to take two short tests, involving about 30—minutes of your time. Though these are primarily research instruments you will be free to discuss them with your counselor if you so decide. After this period you may continue with your counselor if you both agree it is appropriate. In summary, your participation consists of: (1) making a commitment to see your counselor one hour a week for at least six weeks, (2) listing behaviors related to your problem, (3) supplying the names and addresses of two people we may contact in regard to any changes in your behavior 106 107 which they have observed. and (4) taking two short tests after the sixth counseling session. We want to emphasize that your willingness to participate in no way affects your seeing a counselor at the Center. Our most important consideration is to assist you with the resolution of the problems that stimulated you to seek our help. We do want you to know however, that through the participation of students like yourself we are able to im- prove our effectiveness. Sincerely, David W. Cabush Intern Counselor Dr. Cecil L. Williams Associate Professor and Research Sponsor P.S. If by chance more students volunteer for the study than are needed, those not participating will be notified and assigned to a counselor according to the normal procedure. APPENDIX E SUBJECT CONSENT FORM MICHIGAN STATE UNIVERSITY East Lansing, Michigan 48823 Counseling Center Student Services Building CONSENT FORM Code Name Code Number I, , student number , agree that information obtained during the course of my counseling and testing sessions may be used for scientific purposes. This permission covers the use of test results, counselor reports, sound recordings, and behavior ratings given by the individuals I have identified for that purpose. This permission is given with the understanding that all information will be treated in a professional manner and that adequate safeguards will be taken to insure anonymity. Signed Date 108 APPENDIX F "PART I"--PRE-EXPERIMENTAL DATA FROM SUBJECT Code Name Code Number PART I Section A Think of the various ways that the problem for which you are seeking help affects your behavior. . . . Put an- other way, think of those behaviors which you would like to change in some way. Now, make a list of what you would hope to be doing differently as a result of counseling? These behavior changes should be meaningful to ygg and likely to be observed by others. Please list as many as you can, but at least five. A good example would be: "Talk more frequently and openly with my roommate about my feelings." A pggg example would be: "Feel better about my room— mate." (The first is observable behavior, the second may not be.) 1. () 2. () 3. () 4. () 5. () -—minimum-— 6. ( ) 7. () 8. () Next, look over your list and rank each behavior according to its relative importance to you when compared with the others. Do this by first identifying that which you consider most important and placing a "l" in the paren— thesis to the right, then a "2" to the right of that which is second in importance—-and so forth until you have ranked the entire list. --GO ON TO THE NEXT PAGE-- 109 110 PART 1, Continued Code Name Code Number Section B As indicated in the letter given to you by the counselor, we are asking you for the names and addresses of two individuals who we may contact after you have had about six sessions with your counselor. These individuals will be asked to respond to a brief checklist which will indicate behavior changes they may have observed during Winter Term. They will ESE be told that you are seeing a counselor, only that you are participating in a research project and have given their names for the purpose of the checklist. (You will also have a chance to respond to the checklist.) When choosing these names please consider the fol— lowing: --Person should be in a position to have firsthand knowledge of your behavior during Winter Term. --To your knowledge the person will be willing to cooperate when asked to complete the checklist. Name Address (may be omitted if accurately listed in Student Directory) Name Address *Name Address *If you are doubtful as to the availability or will— ingness of one of the above, please give us an alternate. It is important for the success of this project to receive responses from Egg individuals. Again, we thank you for your cooperation! You will be noti— fied soon about your counseling appointment. PLEASE RETURN TO TEST OFFICE SECRETARY APPENDIX G COUNSELOR GUIDELINES FOR TRAINING IN FACILITATIVE SELF-RESPONDING COUNSELOR GUIDELINES FOR TRAINING IN FACILITATIVE SELF-RESPONDING David W. Cabush Michigan State University Counseling Center 111 112 RATIONALE If one assumes that a person seeking counseling is in fact manifesting his inability to solve some problems independently, then it follows that a central endeavor of counseling should be an improvement in the counselee's ability to help himself. This suggests that there is: (1) an underlying problem solving or helping process that is applicable across a range of counselee problems, and (2) this process is amenable to predictable change. The helping process upon which this study focuses is a modification of the Carkhuff model for facilitative interpersonal function— ing. The method for effecting change will be a direct training procedure for counselees. Facilitative Interpersonal Functioning Virtually every personality theorist has attended to the nature of early interpersonal relationships, seeing them as at least partially predictive of adult psychological functioning. From early childhood an individual's emotionaL intellectual, and physical development can be viewed as having been dependent upon: (1) a "first person" (parents, teachers, counselors), (2) a "second person" (child, stu- dent, counselee), and (3) contextual variables (in large part the conditions offered by others in the environment). These first and second person roles also become defined as more knowing—less knowing, and helper-helpee. 113 The issue of adequacy or quality of these inter- personal, helper-helpee, relationships has been a primary consideration of theorists and researchers. Carkhuff and associates have considered these relationships in terms of various helper and helpee process variables. The primary helpee variable has been self—exploration. The helper variables can be divided into: (1) helper—responsive con— ditions, and (2) helper—initiated dimensions. In this con— text, effective processes are broken down into two general components: (1) understanding and, (2) action. Within each component critical variables have been delineated. Understanding: (l) empathy, (2) regard, (3) gen- uineness,* and (4) concreteness.* Action: (1) confrontation, and (2) immediacy. *Can be considered "swing dimensions." According to the developmental phases of helping they may be either responsive or initiative. At this juncture it is important to note that the above process variables have been shown to be directly related to various outcome variables. These outcome cri— teria range from paper and pencil psychometrics to direct behavioral correlates ot helpee problems. Though these outcome variables are not the primary concern of this phase of the study, it should be kept in mind that changes in the above process variables does affect covert and overt be— havior in a predictable manner. 114 This study is directly concerned with the “under— standing" component of helping, thus the "core“ facilitative dimensions-—empathy, regard, genuineness, and concreteness. When considering these dimensions the concept of "level" is crucial. The facilitative dimensions are not some static quality that a helper mysteriously has, or has not. Rather, they can be seen as being operational at some level in any helper-helpee interaction. Also, "minimally facilitative" levels of these dimensions have been established. Numerous experimental and correlational studies have shown 3.0 on a 5—point scale to be minimally facilitative. We may say in general that level 3 of all dimensions represents or establishes an openness or readiness to respond at the higher levels. Only at the higher levels, however, is it possible ultimately to make a difference in the lives of the helpees. "Minimally facilitative," then, refers to the minimal level of conditions in which an effective and viable communica- tion process of helping can take place (Carkhuff, 1969). Another way to view the meaning of "minimally facil— itative" is to consider it as that level of communication where the helper can hear and respond to the helpee without significant distortion. It is reflective in the most basic sense. Also, an examination of the scales for each of the dimensions further clarifies the concept of level. Training on the Facilitative Dimensions Systematic training programs have been developed for improving the interpersonal functioning on the dimen- sions of E, R, G, and C. Such training programs have 115 followed three modalities to date: (1) training helpers, (2) training significant others, and (3) training helpees directly within an interpersonal context. The evidence is continuing to mount for training as an effective and even preferred mode of treatment. But there remains another mode of training which has yet to be systematically explored, that of training for ef— fective helping with oneself. Research has clearly shown that laymen can be trained to function at higher levels of the facilitative dimensions interpersonally, so we have reason to expect that a motivated helpee can be trained to function at higher levels with himself. The most direct form of training as treatment, then, is to train the client himself in the skills which he needs to function effectively. The culmination of such a program is to train the client to develop his own training program. To say, "Client, heal thyself!" and to train him in the skills necessary to do so is not only the most direct—-but it is also the most honest and more effective—-form of treatment known to man (Carkhuffl 1970). The goal of this study then is to train helpees to assume effective helping roles with themselves. In other words, we will attempt to improve both qualitatively and quantitatively a client's repertoire of verbal responses to his own problems. This will mean increased levels of E, R, G, and C for his own helping responses, and an increased number of alternative responses for any given problem. 116 TRAINING FOR FACILITATIVE SELF-RESPONDING Effective training incorporates the (1) modeling, (2) didactic, and (3) experiential sources of learning for shaping the facilitative level of trainee responses. You, as counselor-trainee, will (1) model high level responses at various phases of your interaction as helper, (2) tgggh the elements of facilitative responding—-E, R, G, and C-- and the client will (3) experience the effect of your re- sponses and practice responding to his own problems while receiving feedback (selective reinforcement) from you. The ability to discern a high level (above 3.0) from a low level response has been found to be a prerequi— site for, but not a guarantee of, the capacity to initiate a high level response. Therefore, effective training has been divided into two phases: (1) discrimination training, and (2) communication training. The trainee must be taught to recognize a high level response—-to discriminate-~before he can be expected to communicate or initiate one. The training procedure for our six-week study can be broken down into three sequential phases which outline the general direction and substance of training. These phases are a necessary guide, but are not mutually exclusive aspects. Trainees will differ in terms of the amount of time different phases will take; the time indicated is meant as a general guide. 117 Training Outline I. II. Introduction (session I) A. Listen to, and communicate understanding of, the client's presenting problem, i.e., estab— lishing the relationship. This is a crucial factor in client motivation as well as an opportunity for you to be "facilitative." It is at this point where the client will get most of the answer to that question he's been think— ing about for days--"Will this counselor care about me as much as he does his research?" Provide client with the rationale and goals for training in facilitative self-responding. Feel. free to use the earlier sections of this guide to prepare your rationale. It is important that the client believes you think this will definitely be helpful for him with his particu— lar problems. Deal with any specific problems related to the client's participation-—motivation, questions, doubts, schedules, etc. Discrimination Training (sessions 1 — 3) A. Practice gross rating (good, fair, poor) of responses on the SHeS & HrR tape, with you giving feedback; focus on feelings and empathy. Make a didactic presentation of facilitative dimensions (2nd session). Provide the client 118 with a written description of the dimensions and their levels to study independently between 2nd and 3rd sessions. C. Model high, medium and low levels of E, R, G, and C in self-responding using the SHeS & HrR. D. Practice rating levels of E, R, G, and C in self-responding using SHeS & HrR, with you giving feedback. III. Communication Training (sessions 4 - 6) A. Practice responding in helper role to SHeS (PS 14 - 24), with you giving feedback. B. Have client role play helper and helpee roles with you, focusing on various dimensions of E, R, G, and C as needed. Have client use real problem statements. C. Have client role play with self. You should give periodic feedback designed to shape in- creasingly higher levels of E, R, G, and C in the self—help responses. Use the tape recorder so you can discuss together various responses. ADD IT IONAL NOTE S Of the four facilitative dimensions empathy (E) is the most important. Research has shown that the other variables interact with this one. In the context of this study, E relates to a client being "in touch" with his own feelings, or "owning of feeling." 119 Training per se should be done for a minimum of forty minutes within each session. Therefore, you may use twenty minutes for working in other ways with the client. The client has been assured our primary consideration is him. At some point clarify with the client that this may be only the first phase of counseling (for some it may be enough). After six sessions you can decide together on future direction. You may wish to see the training as a phase one in counseling where the client learns to be a good client—-getting in touch with and accurately labeling his feelings and other elements of his problems. If you cannot meet at a regular appointed time, please try to make arrangements for another hour within that same week. It is important the client gets a minimum of six sessions before March 9th. Use Room 203A, the interview room in the Research Wing, for counseling. Sue will take care of setting up tape if she knows in advance. If a problem comes up with scheduling that room, my office has also been set up for multiple recording. APPENDIX H TYPESCRIPT FOR STANDARD HELPEE-- HELPER STIMULUS TAPE hmusmo mcflammcsoo wnflmnm>flco mumum cmmHQOHE Smsnmo .3 UH>MQ mmmcommmm DDOQDHB vmlea mpcmEmDMDm Emagoum mmmcommmm QDHB maaa mnemamumnm Emaboum mam/NB mDADZHBm mmmqmmlmmmqmm Qmflm umsh 50% we Hmppwn Hmmw p.50m wusm wu.50> pan Om m.ufl meHumEom .mcflammm hamcoH CEBU m m.Dm5D paw .50> DSOQM mumo on Snow u.cop wamowm "m mmcommwm .DH £DH3 meow on can mmummp H50» mcflpumm usonm co ow .pomm mean CH mcoam Dos 0M.50> umnu we comm pmsfl ma op on come 50% ans: .SHHMHUOm who: pesonm wEHu boson m mcfl>ma comb m>.so> om u< oncommwm .Hoosom DSOQDAB DH mcflhnu paw mefibu maosz map mcflxosgo mxfla Hmww H moEHquom .mmHm upon Imam pdonm musclnmnmo on mEmmm haamms Seeboc pew mummw 03p umoEHm 30s who: coon w>.H mxfla rsosx sow “mum: venous mmsoH 0m mem H H BZMEMB0H 50> maosx 50> mo puma no mmcommmm .mchmuumsHH sump m_uH pew .mmcHHmmm meoupmllmpcmumm H50» usonm mmsHH00H UmxHE m>me sow "m omeommmm .muw so» 0Mm£3 Ewan Hku p.20t can 0H0£3080m ucmfipummm cm ODCH m>OE swap pew HHOImsb on Soap HHoB .xmmun emmHo m mme pmsfl mH 0p on p00: 50» pan: "d mmcommwm .mng on Hmzmsw 050m on on now m.mum£u paw .wmuoz cmunom mHHmmH m_uH .BOQM u.cop H .pcmxom3 >Hm>0 0&0: meHEOO >8 udonm mcHnu mHn m mme can #0H m we HHmo hon“ 30c usn .msmEmo so ©0>HH pew uso pm>ofi H cmn3 uswuwm IHHp on UHSOB DH phononp H .gosE oou wumo moan mammfi mama 080m sH nmzn pmdn m.DH “may mwmsm H uzm .wHHEmH mmoHo m WHHme 0H_03 QOmHummEoo 2H “map haw pHsoo 50> wwwsm H .nomw :H .DSOQM Bosx H mncmumm may m0 080m waH Doc hHHMHowmmm .umcp oxHH meHnuwcm Ho .ms 05 Ummmmfi hHHmmH u.cm>m£ mpcwumm m2 mMBOZ N BZWZHBfiBm qumomm 123 mmBOZ .mHmmma 03D QDHOB D0: m.DH .mcHMMD 0H.30> mmmHo cho 05D mH mDHme HH mm Dom paw pHHOB 0HDDHH c30 DHmnD mH H0 MGHSD hme HHm 0xHH mpcsom mmpcmamp DanmIMIDso £05m 0MME 0&3 zonwcm mwoum 0m0£D 0H0 0:3 "0 0mQOMmmm .mHHSBM DOM DH DmmHOH Dmsfl mH 0p 0D town 50% Dan: .Dso MH03 HHm HHHB DH .cme was 30c Domm DmnD ODQH Dwm mHmowm Dmoz .DamHD as 0m Dom D.coo "m oncommmm ”camp m nDuoz cm>m Dos m.DmnDIIDH5g ODIIDDO msoH>bo one .HHmz 0bD CD as Umxomn mcHDDmm How HHmm IDDOH bDHB mSOHHSH 0D.sow "e oncommmm mmchDwD0>m Dw “Ho ummmam E.H inn mama .DH aha: HHma on hum pHsoo H bmHS Dmsfl H .mcop xnos DmnD HHm Dom meHmmom D_cm0 H ”M003 Dxmsllms mcHEOO 0nd mmem was .msp mummmm UHQSDm 03D Dom 0>.H 30c tam mmo mmcHnD Dam 0>_H 0xHH .Boax so» .op 0D Dms3 Song D.sop H .£< m Bzm2m949m Endmomm 124 mMBOZ .UCMDm 50» men3 305x 0D 6002 »HHM0H 50» D59 .EHQ 0D DQMDHOQEH mH £0Hmmm0H mHQ Bocx 50»I|50» D5090 0H00 0D GOmHmm mHSD D503 50» Bocx 0p 50» meoxm .D503 50» D033 m.DH QHMDHmo Dos 0H.50» tam .msHHmme500 EH meomm 1x0 50» DMQB D_smH mHQB "o wmsommmm .SonmmmH sHmp mHn cam 50» cmszmn onoso m mme 0D H0H0m2500 DmnD HHmD 0D D£m50 50» 0MHH mpc50m DH "m 0mGOMm0m .DHsa on Hmemm on Dm5fl UH503 DH D5Q .mHmmH Honms5oo H50» DMQB D50Qm 0HOE Bocx 0D mxHH p.50» .oGHHOm5500 H50» D5020 CHMDHO0Q5 0H.50» tum mmcHHmmm poxHE 0>ms 50» "< 0mCOMm0m .30cx D.cop Dm5fl H .mchD»H0>0 pew aonmmmH mHn DDHB »m5n m.w£ .30£»sm 0EHD m.H0H0wC500 03D mcHDmmz Dm5fl »Hnm90Hm E_H .mHHbsm HOH DH DomHOH 0D HmDDmb 0n UH503 DH 0b»mE 0xHH .0H08»5m 0H5m Doc Dm5n E.H paw DH D50Qm OQHHGHQD 500D 0>_H .D5m .D5o meHOm mmcHnD 080m Dom mmwoon 03D cH 30£0E0m pew .mHmo UH503 D£m50£D H Dng .Bocx 50» .OD RHMD 0D »©0b0§0m common »HHme H mm5m0. @CHmeC500 How 2H wEmo H v BZWEHBHH0 0H0E £058 OmIIH5HprQOB 000 mcHDHoxm mH £0H£3||H00H 50» 30h 0ms0£0 D.smmop D0£D D5n .pc0DmHmpc5 D0: »0E mHmnDO "d 0mcomm0m .memem 305x 50>0 D.5©Hp H 0H0£D D50 UHHOB H5HHD500Q 300 0H0£3 0 m.0H0£D 0xHH Hmmw H .DH »0m 0D »03 HmsDo 0c Dm5fl m.0H0£Duu0>0H CH 3058 On 0H.03 .H 000 mtz .D00Hm »HHO0H m.DH .DcmHmmep Om H00H HIIMBOBIID0£D D05“ m.DH .D.c00 mpcwHHm »E H0 DmOE 0MHH IUQ0DmepC5 »HH00H n00 weo»c0 Db5op H mMBOZ m BZWEMB0 CH mCOHDm m.»pogoz .0NHH00H 5o» C0CD DC0D I0QE00 0HOE 0H.50» IHH0m no 0mC0Mm0m .00 0D DC03 50» C08 0CD mCH0Q 0D mH 0H0CD HH0 D_CmH D0CD D50 .m0p0Hm 0CD CDHB DH 0M0E C00 50» 30Cx 50» .HH0Em »DD0HQ mH00H D0CD UCO .DO0QEH C0 0M0E D_C00 5o» 0xHH HO0H 50» »HHOH000 UCO »HH00Hm»CQ mCHDOmEOO 0D w0E00 DH C033 ”m 0mCOMm0m .DH CDHB m5lp0m »DCmHE mCHDDOm 0H.50» UC0 m00H0 H0H0>0m CH H0mCHHDm|©C000m 0 0xHH H00H 50» 0xHH mUC50m "m 0mC0Mm0m .BOCM D.Cop Dm5n »0CD II0CHH Dm5fl mCHOU E.H xCHCD m©C0HHH »E 0C0 mxHOH »E .om .xHOB poom UHU .m0p0Hm @000 Dow .DC0p5Dm @000 0 C000 m»03H0 0>.H MmUH03 IH0DH0 »DDHCm H00H H .000 .D00m m.H0>HH© 0CD CH Q5 m©C0 C0>0 »HH05m5 HHHq 0CD .HH03 .0D0p 00 H C0C3 UCC .»5m H0CD0 0800 »b D50 D00b D0m 0D E000 C0>0 »HH05m5 HIImCHCD»H0>0 0C0 m0D0U OCHDD0WIIm5mE00 C0 0H0: DCmHH m.DH .H0CDH0 mDHomm DOC Dm5fl m.DH D5m .pHp H C0CD C0DH0 0H0E 0HDDHH 0 »0Hm 0D poom DH0H 0>0C UH503 DH D59 .mCHCD»C0 H0 HODm H0m5m .Hz 00 0D DC03 D.C0© H .mCHHDm 0C000m D5000 U0UC0 m»03H0 H D59 rHH0Q0m0Q 0C0 HH0QD00H HOH D50 DC03 H Hoosom CmHC CH C0>0 .HH03 m EZMEmB¢Bm qumomm 127 .C00H0m C30 H50» 09 0D 0>09 50» .00CODOHx0 H50» HOHDCoo D.C00 »09D .HHO D00 0D 0H093 809D HH0D 50» D_Cop »93 m»HmC0 00 50» 0908 D09D 0HQO0Q 0009D 0H0 093 "U 00C0m00m .D0» 0H0 »HH00H 50» 093 0H50 DOC 0H.50» D59 .H0mC0H »C0 09 D.C00 50» 093 0H50 ©0C80p 0H_50» "m 0090m00m .»03 0800 09D CH C00 H0mCoH 0C D59 ‘00 0D DC03 50» meH9D Ho DoH 0 0H0 0H09B .DoH 0 p0mC090 0>.50» D09D 30C9 50» "0 00C0m00m .00DC03 »09D D093 D05flu|09 0D 08 p0DC03 p30H0 09D D093 003 H .9Dm0p 0C 009 H .08HD D09D D0 003 H 093 003 D09D D5m .Ho U50Hm 09 0D mCH9D0800 003 D09D 9CH9D 0D 0005 H =.»po9»H0>0 9DH3 mCOH0 0D0m 090 .Hooo 090 D.CmH= .09HH 00CH9D »00 0D 0005 0090HHH »8 HHC .»009»H0>0 9DH3 mCoH0 D00 0D 0005 H C093 08HD 0 003 0H09D 30C9 50» .H0H0HHH0Q50 CD 08000 HH0 DH .00000Hm I00 0809 0800 H 0C0 .D0005 D0m H .0H08»C0 00800 0HDDHH UHQ5D0 HH09D »0Hm 0C0 0NHHOH000 0D 8000 D.COU H .0Hm00m 80HH mCH30HU :aDHs 0Hmmms 08H0 H 050 .00 H >03 090 08H |H00H mC0H3 Ho D9mHH 8.H HH 30C9 D_Cop H m MBOZ \l EZMEMBANB m qumomm 128 mMBOZ .»009 0 09HH mCHH00H D05fl C09D 0>0H EOHH 000C 0C0 DC03IIDC03 50» D09D 0H08 0.0H09D D09D 30C9 00H0 50» D59 .»HH05x00 C0803 0 09HH H00H 0C0 D00 C00 50» 30C9 50» no 00C0m00m .»Dm80 wCHHO0H 5o» 00>00H D09D 0C0 .QH90C0HD0H0H H5O» Ho D50 00H0 9058 D00 0D 0H09 0.DH D09D DHOQ DC0CH800 0 9050 080009 009 x00 "0 00C0M00m .0HH930 How 8H9 9DH3 mCOH0 om 0D 0>09 D05fl HH_50» C0H00H 09 309 0C0D0H00C5 C00 50» .mCH9D 0 0H9 D09D »HH00H DOC 0.DH .HH09 90 "C 00C0M00m .0H08»C0 0>0H »9 C008 03 D093 0H50 C0>0 DOC 8_H D09D 8H9 9DH3 mCH9D 0H9 0 9050 09 OD cmuDom 0.DH imsHD 09D HHm Doc DmsHIIDH 030C9 09 9CH9D H 0C0 O0 »HH00H H .x00 »OnC0 D.CO0 H D09D DOC 0.DH .HH03 .8H9 9DH3 9000 09D ODCH H30H0 0D DC03 0_H 8H9 00>OH »HH00H H HH D09D H09D0C0 H0 »03 0C0 90DHm 09D mCH908 8H9 9DH3 m5 00C0 DH 0C0 .0C000 009 »HH00H 0 I0H0009 0H9 0 CH 05 00C0 D05n DH .OD DC03 »HH00H D.Co0 H HH .H09D000D 0H.03 08HD»H0>0 D008H0 r3059 50» 09HH .009 CH 05 0C0 03 000HC5 »0009 DOC D05n 0.09 .»HODOH D5m .8H9 09HH »HH00H H .9DH3 @CHom 8_H »5m 0HCH m BZHEWBflBm EmHmOMm 129 mmBOZ .0000: 00000 000 00000 0.000 000 00 000 0000 .0003 .C08 000090 50» 0C000 09D 0.D09D HH D5m .0H00 009 0800 00QQOH0 0>.50» 09»08 0C0 .900Hm 0 0H.50» 00 .M.O "0 00C0M00m .mCH9D 0H093 09D 05 mCH>Hm D509DH3 .03OH 0009D 0H0>0 0D »03 0 0CHH 0D DC03 50» "m 0090m00m .mCHC0D9mHHH C800 0.D09D DH0m D09D 0_DH 0C0 rDC03 D.C00 50» 30C9 50» mCH9D0800 Dom 00H0 0>.50» D5m .0C000 0H093 09D 80HH DC03 50» @CH9D0800IImCH9D 10800 D00 0>.50» 30C9 50» "0 00Com00m .0C000 009 0 09 »HH00H C00 DH .C08 30C9 D.C00 H 09HH CD59 r9m5OH9D DH 9HO3 08 00mH09 0C0 0C50H0 C009 »HH0505 0>09 00COHHH »E .30C9 50» 0009 mCH900H0 »HH00H 09HH C008 H MC08 303 0MHH .0H08859 H00H 0800 0C00 H 008HD 0Hm500 D00H 09D 0C0 . . »H0D0H HHDC5 H000 »HH00H C009 0.0CH9D I»H0>m .0C000 m5H0 09D 9DH3 »>009 »DDOHQ C009 0>.H D09D »00 0H500 50» 00050 H OH 0 m mBZmSmBfiBm qumomm 130 950» 0D DC0DHOQ8H CH00 0H 90H93 D59 .0C0D0 IH00C5 D.C00 50» 0CH9D0800||0HQO0Q H09D0 9DH3 0CH00H8 C009 0>.50» D09D 0900HH 9DH3 0CH9D0800 0C50w 0>.50» 09HH .0CHmmHHD C09D DH 0D 0H08 0.0H09D .0» no 00COM00m .005H0 090D D.CO0 093 0Hmo0m C0>0 00900HH 09HH 0Hmo0m 00000 0o 00H 0 .00000 0 000 0000 00: 0.00 .0900HH 09HH 50» 00 .9000 "m 00C0m00m .809D 0CH00 0H0 D09D 5o» 0C50H0 0HQO0m 09D 9DH3 005H0 Ho 0D00HH0 09D 0CHD0H00000 DH0D0 OD »000 »DD0Hm 0.DH .0H5DH50950 0H093 09D 0D 005H0 09D 80HH D00 50» 090H9 09D 00NHH0H0C00 »H9090Hm 0>_50» n< 00C0m00m .0HH03 Ho 0CH9 »HH00H 0.DH .30C9 D.Co0 H .0900HH 9DH3 0CH09 0H0 0C0 900HH 0 0CH09 0H0 000H H 00000 0HH000 Hlu000 00003 000 .00000 0H093 09D 0.DH .HH03 .09HH .CO 08 0CH5D D09D HH0 DOC 0.D09D D59 .0CH9D 0C0 0.D09D 0&5I0CHD0090 .0CHQmHHD 09H .HHO D0 08 059 D_Cm000 D09D 0CH9D0800 09HH .00D 0C000 0Ho93 09D D5090 0CH9D0800 0.0H09D 00050 H mMBOZ 005CHDCOU .OH 0 0 mBZHEHB 0>HD000C 0H9D 50» 0CH>H0 .0D050000CH H000 50» 0CH908I|0H09 DH50H D0 0H HHO0DH »DOH000 09»02 "U 00COM00m .HO0H 50» 309 C0 D00 D.C00 50» 0050009 .OOD HH00H50» 9DH3 0.DH11809D D05n DOC 0.DH D50 9050HH5H 50» 0908 »09D .C80D "m 00C0m00m .00 5o» C09D »H0>HD00HH0 0H08 809D 0H0C09 0H500 50» 90H3 50» 0C0 .»H0C0 50» 0908 »HHO0H »099 "a 00C0m00m .D09D 00 D.C00 H 00000 H .000 ”HH09 00 00 00 HH0 E000 HH0D 0C0 809D 9DH3 D00Co9 09 0H500 H 90H3 D05fl H .0H08»C0 809D 0C50H0 09 0D DC03 C0>0 D.Co0 HIIHH00»8 D0 000 0000Hm D00 H 0C0 .HH03 0 Q5 08 0>HH0 »09B 900HC09Q HO 90C59 0 D05n 0H_»09B .0H08»C0 809D 9DH3 0CH09 0C0D0 D_C00 D05fl H m0H0 »09D 9CH9D »09D 00 093 90HmO0m 0009D .C0z mmBOZ HH BszmB0 .90 "0 00Com00m .D.CO0 50» HH HH00H50» 09»08 D59 .000C090 50» HH 0DC0H0m H50» DCHomm00H0 »HH00H 0.50» 09HH H000 50» "m 00C0m00m .CH0DH00 D09D 0H03 5O» 90H3 50» .00 OD 50» D00mx0 0H09D0 D093 H00H0 0H50 0.DH "0 00C0m00m .DOH 0 00D00>CH 0>_»09D .902 .CODDOH 00CH9 HO0H H 0H0m08 0CHOC090 »H9H0000 D5090 9CH9D H C093 C0>m .0H9 0HDDHH 0 003 H 0OCH0 H0>0 H00CH0C0 C0 0CH09 08 Co 0CHH50HH Ho 0CH9 C009 0>_»09D HH03 .09HOH »2 .DHO CH 0H5D5H 0C 0.0H09D 030C9 »009»H0>0 .DH0 ODCH 0CH00 D5090 0>09 H 000H 0H9D D5090 0CH9D I0800 o0 0H500 H 90H3 D05n H .0D 08 0D00mx0 »009»H0>0 09HH .30C9 50» .30C CH 8_H Hofl08 0H9D 90HCHH 0H5O90 »HH00H H 00050 H NH BZWEMB.50» .D5m .00CHH00H H5O» 0CHH000C00 0C0 »CO9m 0CH09 HO 90H0 0H.50» no 0090M00m .»03»C0 0Hmo0m 9DH3 0H95OHD ODCH 50» D00 D05n 00CHHO0H D002 .0Hmo0m OD 00HC 09 0»03H0 OD C5H 0COH 09D CH HMO 0»0Q »HH00H DH D5m "m 00Comm0m .0Hmo0m 9DH3 DO0C09 0CH09 DOC 9DH3 DH 009 »HH00H 0>.50» "H 00C0®00m .0H08»C0 C00 C0>0 H 0H50 DOC 8_H .»009»C0 9DH3 00H0>0H »HH00H 0>_H 0OCH0 0C0H 00 C009 0.DH .0Hmo0m 0D 00HC 0»03H0 8.H 0»00 D09D 0C0 09Dul08HD 09D HH0 9008 0H9D 0CHH003 Ho 00HHD 00 D00 H meOZ MH BZWEHBdBm EMHmomm 134 9000H0 D09D 003 H .C08 303 09HH .30C 0C4 .0CH9D »8 D.C003 90080 C3OC9 0»03H0 0>_H 09HH D59 .08HD 0C0H 0 How 0000 0CH00 C009 0>.H 09HH 0C0 .D90HC D00H m5 0CHDOO90 OD C08 000H0 D09D 0800 H .30C9 5O» mH BZHEMB¢Bm ZWHmomm .H000 00D DOC 0.D09D 0C0 . . . 000H0 0C0»C0 .30C9 50» . . . 0C0»C0 D00 D.C0>09 »HHO0H H 09HH .0HO3 09D 0CHDD00 »HHOCHH 8_H 3OC D59 .00C0HHH mo DOH 0 00000C D09D 0Q»D 09D D.C003 H HH00»8 HH0D 0D 0005 H mmBOZ 0H BZMSWBmH w.H MH .m: “H xoflm v.mg ammon wan pfl um Umucflg mwcflx ®>.H wwwsm H .mHHmmu you psn IImem H 30: usonm mmmemE map EH: m>Hm Op UmHHu m>.H .mGOHw >5m mflsu mcflmcfluum qmwn m>.H wwsmo. %#pH£w 0m mew H PH BzmzmflflBm qumomm .memwummw umm umsn H .HHwBIImucm3 mam mcflaumgw>m auHB mcon monHoH w: 30: gsonm MQHQu H cmaz Bocx 50w .wuwsu umsn m.m£ .HHmn .Umw HE 62¢ .wmso: map masu Ho wcfix 03m WMm UHDOO 50> ~HHm3 mum£#ofi m2 mMBOZ ma BZWEHBfiBm qumomm 136 .mmHm mumnshcm umm u.:0© H mUHM uoom mmonu nuHB maflxuoz mcflnmeOm umm H 30cx #msfl H mu.cow H psm .UHsoo mam ~umpgmn pH wooumumwcs H HH mnhmfi waH ~Bocx p.206 H .mE usonm Ucmumumwas H.Cmmow mgm mafing ch0 map usoam mH mHSB .HmEEdw mflnu Hoogom Op om ou mcflcuMHm uoc gmeHmm mcflmn 8_H mxcflgu >HHmd#om mam K302x sow .wmfluuma pwm Op cmHm m3 umap umumm 6cm ‘Hmmm pxmc mumsomum cmo H 0m Hoonow HmEESm ou om ou ma mucmz mam .Unmpmumvcs p.cmmow #msfl mam ~UG¢HHHHHH® >6 »HHMHommmm .HHmB .mwcmHuH >5 Ho mEOm usm .mumzwm mcflmeU on mcflvmmu maflaomwu Scum mcflnpwum>w 00 mg .mQMHUQH cmOHH®E< Ho mQHHp m cflnuHB mcflxuoz 6cm mQH>HH .302x :0» ~mquESm wan CH Emumowm muMm>om mflng apHB xuoz H HHmB mmBOZ mH BZWEHBm3 mHCp mp UHDOO #H CmHB H .mpMOHCCEEoo $3 um» \mwonHmmmHC COHHHHE m m>mg m3 musm E_H .mE zoCx 50% mxHH Hmmm H umm .50» usonm mCHprCm BOCx wHHmmM #.Cow H .uH CHmmew u.Cmo H ON BZMEdeBm qumomm “CwE QC BOHQ og mpCm3 pmsn wmmg we .pfl usonm xcflgu ow H cm:3 can .600 .pH Cm>o mCHow Ho UwHHC E.H ow ~Hm>o UCm Hm>o Cwmws we CH “H Hw>o om H .uH usonm mep on qu3 Cw>m u_C0U H \uH psonm meu Op qu3 u.COU H .CH Came mmHOZ ma BZWZmfifiBm qumomm 138 mmBOZ .mmOHO HHHmmH On HHHOm @Cm mBOH UCm mCmHC H50 Cuon umnpo Comm CuHB wgmgm Op OHQM mn.w3 30C .30Cx sow .OCHE HO mUCOHMH OOHHMOIOm mmmCu Csz HCHOHMO u.Cmm3 H HH QC pH Bwuom @.H BOCOEOm umnu pHmm w>m3Hm H COC3 .wuowmn mmz pH mxHH pOC m.uH .Op mmOHO >CO> 8_H OMCO 30C memHHH HO OHQCOO m pom m>.H NN BzmzmeHp Cw>m mOEHumEOm CwCu UCm meCHCu usonm HOOH H 30C usonm wuofi ouCH mCHCCu Cmmn w>.H .OHOEmCm szm CEMU m waH CODE Om OCHHOOH #OC E.H .mmmumoum mcflxma E_H xaflgp »HHmmu H gun .ufl mam ou Oflwumm pmofiHm 8_H COmmOu OEOm Com HN BZMEMBfiBm qumomm 139 .mem H 30C HHHum m.pMC# nun ~30C uprmmou HHOB wuwwnm mmCHCO pow O>.H 3OCx H mUCm Op mCflmeCCOO uCMB #.Cmmow OE HO puma pmzu ham Op OE COM mmCmm mCm mMmE pH mwoo vN Bzmzmfiflfim qumomm MOE pmsfl usn .mmHm >UOQ>CM uOC COMO >E pOC CEOE wE uOCIIOE HO mmdmomn Cmmmmn mmCHCu BOCM H van .>Cmom OprHH m m.uH mwmz mEOm CH UCC .BOC pm m.pH OHOCB #OC pmsh m.umau COxHq .OHOE>Cm wa mHuuHH w waH COCE Om HOOH u_COU umdn H .Hsz . . . pCQ K50> Op vuflmz UCCOm Op mCflom »HQMQOMQ mH mHCB MMBOZ MN BZMSWB