. .14.; 1:2 I. .51 a L. 3x5. .2 rt... 5 ,3 5.37:7. ‘I. 11 2th.. fines-Nit, : 3p 5. :54 3. I131... s \4:y.:f.1.~n.. 3.: .53.}. I A... . t. 1: ti? ‘ 3 .3 .y 31:11! .9 .1 1: t... .33.. 3.)...3! 3. I. a! “Vii! =.53.! ..‘.L (:1? t: 1321.! , 1.515 a I}? x. . ‘ .1. .1...) .(I? (.u) wee ~ MICHIGAN STATE UNIVERSI lI/lll/llllIllIIII/lI/lllllIl/Il/l/I/l/I/ll/llllllllllll 31293 01019 8715 This is to certify that the dissertation entitled THE EFFECTS OF POSITIVE AND NEGATIVE COGNITIVE SOCIAL PRIMING ON SELF-SCHEMATA, SELF-EFFICACY, MOOD STATES AND MOTOR PERFORMANCE presented by Richard Ray Albrecht has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for Ph.D. Physical Education and degreein _ . Exerc1se Sc1ence WW Major professor Date November 18, 1993 MSU is an Affirmative Action/Equal Opportunity Institution 0712771 "F— ,_ . __..i_,. LIBRARY Michigan State Unlverslty PLACE IN RETURN BOX to remove this checkout from your record. TO AVOID FINES return on or before date due. DATE DUE DATE DUE DATE DUE Mr..— '1 z F—w 9' 2 19 t" ' m 221% f, MSU Is An Affirmative Action/Equal Opportunity Inaitution awn-Mind. pins—p. 1 THE EFFECTS OF POSITIVE AND NEGATIVE COGNITIVE SOCIAL PRIMING ON SELF-SCHEMATA, SELF-EFFICACY, MOOD STATES AND MOTOR PERFORMANCE By Richard Ray Albrecht A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Physical Education and Exercise Science 1993 ABSTRACT THE EFFECTS OF POSITIVE AND NEGATIVE COGNITIVE SOCIAL PRIMING ON SELF-SCHEMATA, SELF-EFFICACY, MOOD STATES AND MOTOR PERFORMANCE By Richard Ray Albrecht Several cognitive factors have been shown to influence the quality of motor performance. Despite theory suggesting that positive and negative cognitive social priming may impact motor behavior, there has been no attempt to hypothesize and test its direct or indirect role on motor performance. This study addressed these previously neglected areas in the motor performance literature. Specifically, it was predicted that: (a) a positive or negative self-schema would be differentially primed by an unobtrusive exposure to either a positive or negative social situation, (b) information upon which task-specific self- efficacy predictions are based would be selectively and differentially processed depending on the type of self-schema invoked through the priming process, and (c) due to a positive relationship between self- efficacy and performance, subjects in the negative priming condition would perform less well on a subsequent motor task than subjects assigned to the positive priming condition. In addition, it was predicted that exposure to the priming condition would differentially influence mood states which would, in turn, impact performance. Employing a randomized block design, 81 female volunteers with self- schemata that were positive (n = 27), negative (11 = 27) or aschematic (n = 27) toward physical activity were unobtrusively exposed to a videotape depicting either a positive, negative or neutral social 7 ,,, .fifliwwgag, __.- _.--__ 1 interaction between a basketball coach and her team. Pre- and post- manipulation self-schemata, self-efficacy, mood state and performance measures were collected. Findings supported the contention that even brief and indirect exposures to seemingly irrelevant positive and negative social contexts are capable of exerting considerable influence on the activation of self-referent cognitions. Specifically, multivariate analyses revealed that although mood states were unaffected by the exposure to the prime, there was a significant priming effect on subjects' subsequent self-schemata ratings and self-efficacy expectations. Even more important was the unique finding that positive and negative priming can significantly influence the execution of a subsequent motor task. Path analyses, however, failed to support the predicted causal relationships among priming, self-schemata, self- efficacy, mood and performance. Applied and theoretical implications of these findings are discussed. To Mom. Dad and Julie ACKNOWLEDGMENTS There is no way to recognize everyone who contributed to this project. When I think about all the help I've received I realize how fortunate I am to be surrounded by so many thoughtful and caring people. Not only did Deborah Feltz serve as my guidance committee chair and dissertation director, she has been my mentor and friend for the better part of the past decade. I can honestly say that whatever I'm able to accomplish professionally. I owe to Deb. Martha Ewing, like Deb, has also been a positive influence on my professional development for many years. Like everyone who comes in contact with Marty, I've grown to value her insight, technical competence, good-natured wit, but most of all, her friendship. In the summer of 1976, Lawrence Messe did the impossible. He taught a math-anxious psychology undergraduate that statistics were not something to be dreaded but rather, a tool to better understand the world. As a member of my doctoral committee, Larry continued to broaden my awareness of methodological and statistical issues. Galen Bodenhausen introduced me to many of the psychological concepts examined in this dissertation. In fact, the rudimentary work upon which this project is based was a proposal I wrote for Galen's cognitive social psychology class. It was only with his constant encouragement that I was able to develop this rather insignificant proposal into a full-fledged doctoral dissertation. Although my initial contact with William Anderson was as his graduate assistant, he and his wife, Kim, quickly became two of my closest friends. It is not an exaggeration to say this project would V never have been completed without the professional and personal support I received -- and continue to receive -- from Bill and Kim. I only regret I will never be able to repay them for their many acts of kindness. I also have to thank four other members of my OMERAD "family." Josie Csete, Karen Lienhart, Therese deSpelder and Chris Reznich have been my full-time friends and colleagues (and part-time therapists). As is the case with Bill and Kim, they have always been there when I needed them. My thanks to each of you. My long-time friend, Sam Ewing also needs to be acknowledged (he just called on the phone and begged me to do so). Seriously, Sam has always been willing to help me in every way imaginable. I couldn't ask for a better friend. I'm only sorry I don't see him as often as I did when we traveled the area giving baseball and coaching clinics. Although the individuals I've mentioned thus far are "like" family to me, I'll only have one true family. My mother, father and sister (and her husband) have always provided me with unconditional love and support. This past year. more than ever, I've come to realize how lucky I am to have them. Unfortunately, my nature doesn't often allow me to express how important they are to me. I trust they know how much I appreciate them. I would also be remise if I didn't mention the direct and indirect contributions of my ex-wife, Karen. I recognize the many sacrifices she made to advance my academic pursuits. Although we will not share the future, we will always share the past. TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES ......................................... x LIST OF FIGURES ........................................ xi 1. INTRODUCTION ...................................... 1 Nature of the Problem ............................... 1 Cognitive social priming ........................... 1 Social schemata .................................. 3 Self-Schemata .................................... 4 The self-schema—self-efficacy relationship ............ 6 The self—efficacy—performance relationship ........... 7 The mood state—self-perception relationship ......... 8 Statement of the Problem ............................ 8 Delimitations ...................................... l 1 Basic Assumptions ................................... 1 1 Limitations ......................................... 12 II. REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE ......................... 13 Cognitive Priming in Social Perception ................. 13 Historical overview ................................ 13 Priming of social judgments ........................ 15 Theoretical models of the priming effect ............. 18 Further delineations of the priming effect ............ 25 Cognitive Schemata ................................. 30 Historical overview of schema theory ................. 30 Schema theory .................................... 32 Self-Schemata .................................... 38 Self-Efficacy ....................................... 45 Mediating (and Mediated) Effects of Mood States ........ 51 The relationship between mood and self-schemata ..... 52 The relationship between mood and self-efficacy ...... 54 Priming of Self-Referent Cognitions .................... 57 Priming of self-schemata ........................... 57 Priming of self-efficacy ............................. 59 Priming of mood states ............................ 61 Cognitions and Motor Performance .................... 63 vii The relationship between self-efficacy and motor performance ................................. 63 The relationship between mood and motor performance ................................ 64 The relationship between priming and behavior ....... 68 III. METHOD ............................................ 75 Pre-Experimental Schematicity Assessment .............. 75 Subjects ........................................... 75 Instrumentation .................................... 76 Perceived Physical Ability Scale (PPA(des)) ............ 76 Importance of Perceived Physical Ability Scale (PPA(imp)) .................................. 76 Marlowe-Crowne Social Desirability Scale ............. 77 Procedure .......................................... 77 Collection of physical ability schematicity data ......... 77 Classification of subjects' self-schemata for physicalability....................... ............. 78 Experimental Data Collection ........................... 81 Subjects ........................................... 8 l Instrumentation .................................... 8 1 Demographic /background questionnaire .............. 8 1 Profile of Mood States (POMS) ....................... 82 Physical ability self-schemata ........................ 82 Task-specific self—efficacy .......................... 83 Golf putting task .................................. 83 Coaching style videotapes ........................... 85 Manipulation Check Questionnaire ................... 86 Procedure .......................................... 87 Collection of experimental data ...................... 87 Experimental manipulation and data collection ........ 89 Manipulation Check Questionnaire and debriefing ..... 9 1 Data entry and analyses ............................. 92 IV. RESULTS AND DISCUSSION ............................ 94 Pre-Manipulation Results ............................... 94 Equivalence of Experimental Group Assignment ......... 94 Equivalence of Schematicity Blocks ................... 96 Assessment of Order Effects .......................... 96 Reliability of Outcome Measures ......................... 97 Reliability of self-schema instruments ................ 97 Reliability of the Profile of Mood State subscales ....... 98 Post-Manipulation Results .............................. 98 Manipulation Check ................................. 98 Effect of Priming on Outcome Variables ................ 100 viii Causal Relationships Among the Variables .............. 1 05 General Discussion of the Results ........................ 1 10 V. SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS ......................... l 17 Summary ............. 117 Conclusions ........................................ 1 2O Implications for Practitioners and Suggestions for Future Research .................................. 12 1 FOOTNOTES .............................................. 124 APPENDICES A Self-Descriptive Version of the Perceived Physical Ability Scale (PPAdes) ...................... 125 B. Self-Importance Version of the Perceived Physical Ability Scale (PPAimp) ...................... 126 C Shortened Version (Form C) of the Marlow- Crowne Social Desirability Scale ..................... 127 D. Demographic/ Background Questionnaire ............... 128 E. Abbreviated Version of the Profile of Mood States ........ 130 F. Alternative Word Lists for the Profile of Mood States ..... 132 G. Golf Putting-Specific Self-Efficacy Scale ................ 134 H. Transcriptions of Manipulation Videotapes ............. 137 1. Description of Coaching Behavior Assessment System (CBAS) .................................... 148 J. Manipulation Check Questionnaire ..................... 149 K. Informed Consent Form .............................. 151 L BIODOT Color Reference Chart ........................ 152 M. Calculations for x2 "goodness of fit" test between hypothesized and fully-identified causal models ........ 153 N. University Approval for Use of Human Subjects .......... 1 56 0. Raw Data ......................................... 1 57 LIST OF REFERENCES ..................................... 175 LIST OF TABLES TABLE Page 1. Postulates Related to the Storage, Search and Retrieval Processes Underlying the Priming Effect . . . 20 2. Conceptual Definitions of Self-Schema .............. 4O 3. Decision Rules Employed in the Classification of Physical Ability Schematicity ....................... 79 4. Descriptive Statistics for All Pre-Manipulation Dependent Variables ............................. 95 5. Alpha Reliability (Internal Consistency) Coefficients for Six POMS Subscales and Total Mood Disturbance Scores ........................................ 99 6. Descriptive Statistics for all Post-Manipulation Dependent Variables ............................ 102 7. Correlations Among Priming and Pre-Post Difference Scores on All Outcome Variables .................. 108 LIST OF FIGURES Page Classic "iceberg" profile exhibited by elite runners, oarsmen and wrestlers ........................... 67 Golf putting performance task .................... 84 Completely randomized block design with replications (Blocking variable: Physical ability self-schematicity) . . 90 Results of planned orthogonal contrasts among manipulation groups on perceived positive/negative style of videotaped coach .......................... 101 Contrasts between manipulation conditions for pre to post differences in each outcome variable ......... 104 Path cofficients for hypothesized relationships among priming and difference scores on all outcome variables ............................... 107 Path cofficients for the full model relationships among priming and difference scores on all outcome variables ................................ l 1 1 xi CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION N ur f Pr 1 m A number of cognitive factors have been shown to influence the quality of motor performance. The sport psychology literature is replete with investigations examining the manner in which cognitive variables such as anxiety, attention, causal attributions, goal intentions, motivation, mood states and self-efficacy contribute to the acquisition and performance of motor skills. Despite the strong theoretical assumption that another cognitive factor -- positive and negative cognitive social priming -- may also exert a considerable influence on motor behavior, there has been no attempt to hypothesize and test its direct or indirect (via its impact on other cognitive mechanisms) role on motor performance. Qegnigve eeeial priming. Although the extent to which cognitive social priming can influence the acquisition and performance of motor tasks has yet to be examined, the priming phenomenon has been the focus of extensive research within the broader confines of cognitive and social psychology. According to the definition set forth by Fiske and Taylor (1984), cognitive priming relates to "the effects of prior context on the interpretation and retrieval of information . . . priming ie specifically the name for the fact that recenm and frequently tiv i e come to mind m re asil ani eas tha hav not W" [italics added] (p. 231). In a frequently cited investigation into the effects of cognitive social priming on impression formation, Higgins and his colleagues (Higgins, Rholes & Jones, 1977) found that interpretation and retrieval of information was even 1 susceptible to priming by seemingly irrelevant prior contexts. In their study. subjects were unobtrusively exposed to a series of either "positive" or "negative" personality traits (e.g., adventurous versus reckless: self—confident versus conceited, etc.). Later, in a supposedly unrelated context, the subjects read ambiguous descriptions about an individual named Donald who engages in such behaviors as mountain climbing, skydiving and demolition derby driving. Despite the fact that ostensibly, the two contexts (exposure to the personality traits and reading the story about Donald) had nothing to do with one another, subjects who had earlier been "primed" through exposure to the positive traits evaluated Donald more favorably than subjects "primed" with negative traits. A similar finding has recently been reported in a sport context. Warm and Branscombe (1990) found that. relative to subjects who had been primed by exposure to nonaggressive sports, subjects exposed to aggressive sports were more likely to rate an ambiguous person as hostile and as being an individual who enjoyed aggressive activities. Fiske and Taylor (1984) offer the following explanation for these somewhat surprising results: "The priming effect suggests that, in the selection of a person schema to apply to the interpretation of new information, those recently activated are more accessible" (p. 175). Similarly, Wyer & Srull (1980. 1981, 1986; Srull & Wyer, 1979) account for the effects of priming by drawing an analogy to a "storage bin" in which those objects (in this case, a positive or negative schema) which are placed at the top of the heap (invoked most recently) become those most readily accessible. Simply put, as the need to process and interpret new information arises. we search our 3 cognitive storage bin -- starting from the top -- for a framework or schema to serve as a perceptual filter of reality. Upon locating a plausible schema (usually one that has been recently and / or frequently invoked), the cognitive miser within us, in its pursuit for economy of mental functioning, curtails the further search for an alternative, and perhaps more appropriate schema. $291911 echemata. The most influential models set forth to explain the mechanisms underlying the priming effect rely on the existence of abstract memory structures or social schemata. Social schemata assist the individual in classifying and categorizing new social information because every second of our waking lives we, as human beings, find ourselves bombarded by literally thousands of pieces of information pertaining to the social and nonsocial world around us. Although we are fortunate in that the vast majority of these stimuli can be safely ignored, there are always certain pieces of information impinging upon us requiring further cognitive processing. With so much information to process and such a limited mental capacity with which to perform this task, we are often forced into the role of "cognitive misers" -- taking mental shortcuts that enable us to make instant judgments regarding the relevance or non-relevance of particular stimuli (Fiske & Taylor, 1984; Wyer & Srull, 1986). Although these cognitive shortcuts or heuristics are essential in dealing effectively and efficiently with vast amounts of information, they carry with them the heavy price tag of being inherently prone to processing errors and biases. One form of mental shortcut receiving considerable attention in the cognitive social psychology literature involves the development and use of "social schemata." In the broadest of possible terms, social schemata may be defined as cognitive structures which serve as abstracted or generalized knowledge structures regarding specific social events or entities. Emphasizing the economic necessity of employing such schemata, Fiske and Taylor ( 1984) see them as "organized generic prior knowledge [which] enables us to function in a social world that otherwise would be of paralyzing complexity" (p. 149). Because these abstractions are used to keep us informed as to "the way things are" in the social world, they tend to selectively guide (and in some cases -- misguide) our perceptions, memories and inferences regarding incoming social stimuli. In the words of Markus and Smith (1981) "Schemata are the basis of the selectivity that is operative in information processing" (p. 240). Neisser (1976) even more succinctly summarizes the role of schemata in information processing as follows: "Perceivers pick up only what they have schemas for and willy-nilly ignore the rest" (p. 80). Self-Schemata. Although it is possible to identify several subcategories of social schemata on the basis of their content, such as those used to process and categorize information pertaining to (a) other individuals (person schemata); (b) appropriate norms and behaviors of broadly defined groups of individuals (role schemata); or (c) particular situations or events (event schemata), the primary concern at present is with the specific subset of social schemata that guide individuals in the way they perceive, remember and make inferences about themselves (self-schemata). A common theoretical assumption is that one tends to view the "self" in much the same way he or she views others (Kihlstrom & Cantor, 1984). This general position is expressed by Kihlstrom and Cantor as follows: The question naturally arises as to whether the mental representations [schemata] of oneself differ in some way from one's representations [schemata] of other people. From a structural point of view, we think the answer is clearly no: The self-concept is organized along the same lines as concepts representing others. (p.29) This view that the formation of mental representations about the self are generally subject to the same principles associated with the formation of mental representations about other people led Higgins *- and his colleagues to hypothesize the following: "Given that category accessibility [priming] can affect how a person characterizes another person, it may also affect how a person characterizes his or her own behavior and internal processes" (Higgins, et al., 1977, p.152). Self—schemata, as is the case with social schemata as a whole, enable the individual to form an abstracted or generalized version of a social entity, which is then used as a perceptual filter through which incoming information is subsequently processed. One important aspect of self-schemata is that unlike other types of social and nonsocial schemata, self-schemata are used as perceptual filters for information concerned with the most important of social entities -- the "self." Several theorists. in fact, have gone so far as to define self- concept as nothing more than "a system of schemata" (Markus & Smith, 1981, p. 242) or "a complex, person-specific, central, attitudinal schema" (Greenwald & Patkanis, 1984, p. 130). Such views imply that several different self-schemata may be included within an individual's concept of his or her "self." For example, a particular individual may have a number of equally plausible self-schemata: "me as 6 introvert," "me as musician," "me as athlete," "me as honors student," etc. The specific self-schema that becomes activated, and therefore used as a framework for the interpretation of subsequent stimuli, is simply a function of which schema is most salient at a particular time (Markus 8: Smith, 1981). Several factors such as motivations, relevancy, frequency of activation, or recency of activation may contribute to which, among the several possible self-schemata available, is most salient at a particular time (Fiske & Taylor, 1984; Higgins, Bargh & Lombardi, 1985). Closely related to this principle that self-schemata can be made more salient by frequent or recent activation is the notion that self-schemata may actually be susceptible to social "priming" (Higgins, et al., 1977 ; Higgins et al., 1985). flhe eelf-eehema — eelf-effieaey relaflenehip. A major premise of schema theory holds that self-schemata, by selectively encoding. storing and retrieving information, serve to guide individuals in the way they perceive, remember and make inferences about themselves. Simply put, "when people are asked to predict their own behavior, they usually make predictions consistent with their self-schema" (Fiske & Taylor, 1984, p. 157). Accordingly, this should also include making predictions based on one's self-efficacy beliefs. Markus (1977), in her initial (and highly influential) investigation of the role self-schema play in information processing, found that "schematic" and "non-schematic" individuals differ significantly in the way they process information. Not only did she find self-schemata facilitate individuals’ judgments and decisions about themselves, they further "previde a, paeie fer the eenfigent eelf-prediefien ef behavier en eehema-relateg dimeneiene" [italics added] (p. 63). 7 In a similar investigation into the way in which behavioral self- predictions can be influenced by one's self-schemata, Kendzierski (1988) classified 66 college undergraduates as having either (a) a self- schema for exercising (exerciser schematics); (b) a self-schema for not exercising (nonexerciser schematics); or (c) a self-schema which favored neither exercising nor non-exercising (aschematics). Each subject rated 12 pairs of behavioral alternatives (one of which reflected a pro—exercise orientation, the other alternative did not) on a lOO-point scale indicating the likelihood they would behave in the way described. Results indicated that exerciser schematics thought they would be more likely to choose the exercise-oriented alternative than did either aschematics or nonexerciser schematics. In addition, aschematics were more likely to predict they would choose the exercise—oriented alternative than the nonexerciser schematics. Thus. although the self-schema—self—efficacy relationship has not yet been directly tested, evidence exists that indicates these variables should be positively related. The self-efficacy — performance relationship. The relationship between self-efficacy, which according to Bandura (1977) "is the conviction that one can successfully execute the behavior required to produce [an] outcome" (p. 193), and motor performance has become axiomatic. Not surprisingly, the intuitively appealing notion that task- specific confidence is positively correlated with performance has received overwhelming support from myriad empirical investigations (e.g., Bandura, 1982; Bandura, Adams & Beyer, 1977; Feltz, 1982; McAuley, 1985). Bandura's (1977) original model of the role self- efficacy plays in performance predicts that the relationship between _‘ 8 self-efficacy and performance is reciprocal in that efficacy is the mediator of performance which in turn influences future perceptions of task efficacy. Recently, a respecified model of self-efficacy has been proposed and supported by Feltz (1982, 1988a; Feltz & Albrecht, 1986; Feltz 81 Mugno, 1983) in which both prior performance and self-efficacy act as mediators of subsequent performance. The mead ejate — eelf-pereepgen relatienehip. Observing that individuals clinically diagnosed as depressives tended to selectively focus on past and present negative information about themselves, Beck (1967, 1976) proposed that mood states may be associated with distortions in the processing of information about the self. In addition to providing support for the contention that mood states influence self-relevant information processing, Natale and Hantas (1982) demonstrated that even temporarily elevated or depressed mood states are capable of producing biased memory and information processing. It was found, for example, that having been induced with a "happy" mood state was "associated with decreased recall of negative events and an increased recall of positive events" (p. 927). In contrast, subjects within whom a "sad" mood was induced were found to have a "decreased recall of positive life experiences, weaker memory strength for positive information about oneself, and a bias to recall false negative self-descriptions" (p. 927). m n f Pr m This study was undertaken to address previously neglected areas in the motor performance literature. Specifically, it was predicted that: (a) a positive or negative self-schema would be differentially primed as a result of exposure to either a positive or negative social situation. (b) information upon which task-specific self-efficacy predictions are based would be selectively and differentially processed depending on the type of self-schema that had been invoked through the priming process, and (c) due to a positive relationship between self—efficacy and performance, subjects in the negative priming condition would perform less well on a subsequent motor task than those subjects assigned to the positive priming condition. In addition, it was predicted that exposure to the priming condition would have a direct effect on mood state which would, in turn, impact on performance. Finally, on the basis of Markus' (1977: Markus 8: Sentis, 1982) findings that schematics and aschematics process information differently and possessing a self-schema for a particular behavioral domain makes an individual more resistant to counter-schematic information, it was hypothesized that there would be an interaction between priming condition (i.e., positive, negative) and physical ability self-schematicity (i.e., positive, negative, aschematic). Priming effects, therefore, should be exacerbated when the prime is schema- consistent. Specifically, the following research hypotheses were set forth and experimentally tested using data collected via a completely randomized block design with replications. (1) A negative or "failure-oriented" self—schema will be primed (made most salient and accessible) in subjects who are unobtrusively exposed to a coach punishing (verbally criticizing) her players during a halftime talk. Conversely, a positive or "success—oriented" self—schema will be primed (made most salient and accessible) in subjects who are N‘V‘ 10 unobtrusively exposed to a coach encouraging (verbally praising) her players during a halftime talk. (2) Information upon which task-specific self-efficacy (3 v predictions are made will be selectively and differentially processed depending on the type of self-schema (positive or negative) that has been invoked through the priming process. Specifically, subjects within whom a negative self— schema has been invoked will tend to process subsequent ambiguous information related to a task in a manner consistent with a negative or "failure-oriented" self-schema. In contrast, subjects approaching the task with a positive self-schema will tend to process ambiguity associated with the task in a manner consistent with a positive, "success- oriented" self-schema. Because of these unconscious information processing biases, subjects who overhear athletes being criticized will report lower task—specific self- efficacy expectations for an ambiguous motor task than will those subjects who are made to overhear the coach praising her athletes. Subjects exposed to the positive prime will exhibit significantly lower Total Mood Disturbance (TMD) scores on the Profile of Mood States (POMS; McNair, Lorr & Droppleman, 1971) than subjects exposed to the negative coach videotape. Furthermore, mood disturbance scores will be negatively associated with overall motor performance. As TMD increases, performance will decrease. ‘V‘ —=-—-——'—’* , 1 1 (4) Due to a positive relationship between self-efficacy and performance, subjects in the failure-related (criticizing coach) condition will perform less well on a subsequent motor task than will subjects assigned to the success-related (praising coach) condition. (5) There will be a significant interaction between priming condition and physical ability self-schematicity. Specifically, priming will be exacerbated when it is schema-consistent (i.e., positive schematics will be influenced most by exposure to a positive social prime; negative schematics will be influenced most by exposure to a negative social prime; aschematics will be influenced by both positive and negative social priming). Delimitatione Subjects in this study were limited to college-age females enrolled in undergraduate psychology courses at Michigan State University. All subjects voluntarily participated in the study in return for course credit. Generalizability of the results obtained in the study are, therefore, limited to the specific tasks and subject population employed. B ic m n In addition to having minimal visual and auditory abilities needed to attend to the experimental manipulation, subjects were assumed to possess motivation levels necessary for the accurate completion of all written questionnaires and motor performances. It was also assumed that a 5-minute exposure to the manipulation was of adequate duration and intensity to equally prime negative, positive and neutral self- 12 schemata in each subject. Furthermore, the golf putting task was assumed to be an adequate indication of motor performance. Limi n This study was limited by the inability to make the experimenter unaware of the manipulation to which each subject had been exposed. Although extreme care was taken to treat each subject equally, the lack of a "double—blind" makes it impossible to rule out the unlikely event that some subjects were inadvertently provided subtle positive or negative cues during their interaction with the experimenter. CHAPTER II REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE The present chapter reviews relevant literature pertaining to selected cognitive factors that may, either individually or in an integrated fashion, influence motor performance. Specifically examined are the areas of (a) cognitive social priming, (b) self- schemata, (c) self-efficacy, and (d) mood states. In addition, it presents a synthesis of previous research into the interrelationships among these variables and the influences they exert on performance. i v Primin in i P r n Although the extent to which cognitive social priming can influence the acquisition and performance of motor tasks has yet to be examined, the priming phenomenon has been the focus of extensive research within the broader confines of cognitive and social psychology. A review of these studies is provided as a foundation from which a cognitive social priming research paradigm can be extended into the realm of motor behavior. Hieterieal eveflew. Gardner (1985) has described cognitive science as having both "a very long past and a relatively short history" (p. 9). This statement is equally descriptive of research involving the priming of social cognitions. Social priming's "very long past" is readily apparent by the voluminous priming literature that has been amassed by cognitive psychologists during several decades of research into the effects an initial stimulus (prime) can exert on the response to a second (target) stimulus (Simon, 1988). Also contributing to the "long past" of priming in social cognition are the myriad investigations into the effects of prior social context on subsequently encountered l3 14 social information. Despite the tradition of scientific involvement in both cognitive priming and the effects of prior social contexts on social information processing, only recently has the priming metaphor found its way into the social psychological literature. One indication of just how recently the term "priming" achieved legitimacy is the fact that it failed to meet established criteria for inclusion as a psychological index term until the 5th Edition of the W Pelehelegieal Index Terme published in 1988 (American Psychological Association, 1988).1 A second indication that the use of the priming metaphor within a social context is still in its infancy is the fact that virtually all of the "classic" social cognition priming research conducted in the late 1970's and early 1980's (e.g., Bargh 8r Pietromonaco, 1982; Higgins 8: King, 1981; Higgins, et al., 1977; Srull & Wyer, 1979, 1980) was published with titles and abstracts that were devoid of any reference to priming. Much of this early research into the social effects of priming, therefore, is found under the more conceptually descriptive and still active (e.g., Smith, 1988; Smith & Branscombe, 1987) rubric of category accessibility. A number of early investigations demonstrated it was possible to influence the manner in which social information is perceived, remembered and used in inference making simply by altering the context prior to stimulus presentation. Schachter and Singer, (1962) for example, injected subjects with epinephrine (adrenaline), a hormone known to stimulate the sympathetic nervous system thereby producing a general feeling of physiological arousal. In one experimental condition subjects were informed that the drug may produce an emotional state characterized by increased heartbeat, 15 flushing of the face, sweating, etc. In the other condition, subjects were not told of the physiological effects of the drug. All subjects were then exposed to a social context in which a confederate began acting in an agitated and hostile manner. Subjects who were aware of the drug's efi'ects did not follow the lead of the confederate but those who could make no attribution to the injection tended to act in a manner similar to that exhibited by the confederate. In another study designed to examine the effects of prior context on information processing, Kelley (1950) found that students who were told ahead of time that their new instructor was a "warm" person subsequently rated the instructor more favorably than students to whom the instructor had been described as "cold." Although the results of this study are often cited as evidence of the influence a prior context (the experimenter's labeling of the instructor) can have on the storage and retrieval of social information, Higgins, et al. (1977) offer an alternative explanation for Kelley's findings. They suggest that because the students were aware of the experimenter's personal opinion of the instructor (as being either a "warm" or "cold" person) their public ratings may not have been an accurate reflection of their private judgments or recollections, but rather, the students' conscious or unconscious efforts to provide ratings conforming to the experimenter's judgments. Priming of seeial judgments. Drawing on the previous writings of Bartlett (1932) and Bruner (1957; 1958) suggesting that the perception process involves a linkage between a stimulus and an existing cognitive category, Higgins, et al. (1977) designed a study to test the two equally plausible alternative interpretations (prior context 16 effects versus experimenter demand effects) of Kelley's (1950) findings. In doing so, Higgins and his colleagues developed a paradigm now considered basic to social priming research. Basically, the paradigm involves presenting subjects with a word or series of words (primes) that are related to the trait of interest. Shortly thereafter, and in an ostensibly separate study, the subjects read an ambiguous description of a target person and are asked to form an impression of this person. Priming is said to occur to the extent that the impressions are differentially influenced by exposure to the prime(s). In the Higgins et al. study the investigators unobtrusively (thereby controlling for experimenter demand effects) exposed subjects to a series of "positive" or "negative" personality traits that could be used to describe the same objective behavior (i.e., adventurous versus reckless: self-confident versus conceited; independent versus aloof; persistent versus stubborn). Later, in a supposedly unrelated "reading comprehension" study, the subjects read ambiguous descriptions about an individual named Donald who engages in such behaviors as mountain climbing, skydiving and demolition derby driving. Despite the fact that ostensibly, the two contexts (exposure to the positive or negative personality traits and reading the story about Donald) had nothing to do with one another, subjects who had earlier been "primed" through exposure to positive traits that were applicable to the ambiguous behaviors (i.e., adventurous, self-confident, independent, persistent) evaluated Donald more favorably than subjects "primed" with applicable negative traits (i.e., reckless, conceited, aloof, stubborn). The effect was not. however, solely a function of the positive and negative valence of the 17 personality traits. Subjects exposed to equally positive or negative personality traits that were not directly applicable to making judgments regarding Donald's behavior (e.g., obedient, neat, disrespectful, clumsy, etc.) failed to demonstrate the priming effects. Not only do these findings suggest that exposure to a seemingly unrelated prior context is capable of exerting a powerful influence on the way new social information is encoded, and subsequently used to make judgments about others, they also support the hypothesis that the selection of a memory category to be used in the processing of ambiguous information can be made more readily accessible by recent activation (priming). This preliminary finding that social judgments can be manipulated simply by exposure to seemingly irrelevant prior contexts spawned such a flurry of research activity that priming is now "proven one of the most-studied topics in social cognition" (Smith (Sr Branscombe, 1987 ; p. 490). A host of conceptual replications and extensions of the Higgins et al. (1977) study followed in an attempt to elucidate the nature of the priming effect. Early investigations by Srull and Wyer (1979, 1980) contributed to the priming literature in several ways. First, by assuming that cognitive categories were generalized knowledge structures (schemata) that contained both trait concepts and behaviors, they posited that categories could be made more accessible not only by priming trait terms that presumably represented the category (Higgins et al., 1977) but also by exposing subjects to primes that exemplified those traits. To test this hypothesis Srull and Wyer (1979) had subjects create meaningful sentences from sets of words. Each four-word set was constructed so 18 that only two possible complete sentences could be formed, both describing a behavior either related or unrelated to the trait of interest. In Experiment 1, all word sets formed sentences that were examples of either hostility (e.g., "leg break arm his") or nondescript "filler" items (e.g., "her found know 1"). An additional experiment (Srull & Wyer, 1979: Experiment 2) was also conducted in which the hostile primes were replaced by exemplars of "kindness" (e.g., "the hug boy help"). A second extension of the Higgins et al. study was achieved by manipulating the absolute and relative amount of priming subjects received. Subjects were assigned to one of four conditions that exposed them to either 60 sentences (20% or 80% trait related) or 30 sentences (20% or 80% trait related). Finally, Srull and Wyer manipulated the delay between the presentation of the priming task and the target stimulus. Subjects were asked either (a) immediately, (b) after a 1 hour delay; or (c) after a 24 hour delay, to read a vignette about a stimulus person's (Donald's) afternoon activities. As in the Higgins et al. study, the behaviors depicted in the vignette were ambiguous in regard to the trait of interest (hostility or kindness). When taken as a whole, the results of this study revealed that (a) behavioral exemplars, as well as trait adjectives that are represented within a schema, have the ability to increase category accessibility; (b) category accessibility becomes more pronounced as the absolute and relative number of behavioral primes increase; and (c) although category accessibility decreases with the passage of time, the effects are still detectable as much as 24 hours after priming. Th r c m l f h rirnin ff .Inadditiontothe previously mentioned empirical investigations of cognitive priming on 19 social categorization, several theories have been proposed in an attempt to explain the cognitive mechanisms involved in the priming effect. Wyer and Srull (1980, 1981, 1986: Srull & Wyer, 1979), for example, have developed an elaborate theory of human cognition in social contexts that, among other things, accounts for the effects of priming. Although the entire theory, in its most recent conceptualization (Wyer & Srull. 1986), sets forth 23 underlying postulates, Table 1 presents four postulates regarding information storage and retrieval that are particularly relevant to understanding the cognitive mechanisms underlying the priming effect. The referent bins to which Wyer and Srull (1986) refer in Postulate B—7 can be thought of as an encyclopedia containing representations of "stored knowledge about one's physical and social world" (p. 329). Of particular relevance to the processing of social information are those bins that contain stored knowledge related to persons, groups and events. These referent bins of abstracted social representations of stored knowledge are analogous in many ways to what other theorists have called "social schemata" (Fiske & Taylor 1984). Postulate B-7 simply states that each of these bins has an identifiable "header" that consists of the bin name and a set of salient features associated with the contents of the bin. Searches of social information are conducted by identifying (by means of the header) and searching relevant bins. Upon locating an appropriate referent bin, its contents are searched "from the top down" (Postulate C-9). Once information from a particular bin is selected for use in processing, the original information maintains its position in the bin while an exact copy of the information is sent to the "work space" for use. When the 20 Table 1 . Postulates Related to the Storage, Search and Retrieval Processes Underlying the Priming Effect (Wyer 8: Srull, 1986; p. 336). Peetttlate B-7: The header of a referent bin consists of (a) a name that specifies the referent and (b) a set of features that are strongly associated with it. Peetplate Q-l [Henriette Pestulate]: No more information is retrieved for use in attaining a processing objective than is sufficient to allow the object to be attained. When this minimal amount has been retrieved, the search terminates. P - R n P 1 : When the contents of a bin are searched for the purpose of attaining a processing objective, the search proceeds from the top down. A particular unit of information is identified as potentially relevant if its features include the probe cues governing the search. The probability of retrieving a unit of information from a bin, given that units stored on top of it have not been used, is a constant. Postulate C-lO [Copy Postulate]: When a unit of information in a bin is identified as relevant for attaining a processing objective, a copy of this information is transmitted to the Work Space. Thus, the original position of the information in the bin is preserved. However, when processing objectives are complete, a copy of the retrieved unit of information is returned to the top of the bin from which it was drawn. 2 1 job for which it was copied is complete, the copy is returned to the top of the referent bin from which it was drawn (Postulate C-10). Because a copy of all accessed information is returned to the top of the bin, information that has been most recently activated is stored near the top of the bin and is most likely to be encountered early during the next "top down" search of the bin (Postulate C-9). In addition. because the original information retains its initial position in the bin and identical copies of the information are returned to the bin after each use, a more frequently activated category will have more copies available for retrieval when a particular referent bin is searched. Finally, all search and retrieval processes are guided by heuristics (Postulate C-l). This simply means that an exhaustive search of the bin will not be undertaken and the search will be terminated as soon as enough information is gathered to perform the required task. Simply put, Wyer and Srull (1986) account for the efi'ects of priming (i.e., recently and frequently activated categories are more likely to be used to process subsequently presented information) by drawing an analogy to a "storage bin" in which those objects (e.g., a positive or negative trait category) placed at the top of the heap (invoked most recently) become those most readily accessible. As the need to process and interpret new information arises, we search a relevant cognitive storage bin -- starting from the top -— for a framework to serve as a perceptual filter of reality. Upon locating a plausible framework (usually one that has been recently and/ or frequently invoked), the cognitive miser within us, in its pursuit for economy of mental functioning, curtails the further search for alternative, and perhaps more appropriate information. __x 22 Although Wyer and Srull's (1981, 1986) mechanistic metaphor is widely cited in the literature (Fiske & Taylor, 1984; Ratcliff & McKoon, 1988: Wyer & Gordon, 1984), it is not the only explanation that has been offered for the cognitive mechanisms underlying the priming effect (Ratcliff & McKoon, 1988). Energy transmission models such as those proposed by Higgins, et al. (1985) suggest that: (a) priming serves to increase a construct's excitation level, (b) this excitation level must attain a certain minimum threshold in order to process information, (c) the more frequently a construct is primed. the more likely it is to reach and maintain a useable threshold level of excitation, and (d) the excitation level of a construct will dissipate over time. Just as Wyer and Srull use the storage bin analog to illustrate their model, Higgins, et a1. use "battery" and "synapse" models to depict two energy transmission interpretations of priming effects. In the battery model, the cognitive mechanisms responsible for priming effects are thought to be analogous to an automobile battery. The more a battery is charged (i.e., the more a category is primed) the higher its level of energy (activation) and the more likely its subsequent use. When the battery (category) is not recharged (primed) for an extended period of time, its level of energy (level of activation) dissipates until it is no longer viable when called upon to start the engine (process incoming information). In contrast, the synapse model of energy transmission uses the propagation of neural energy in vertebrates (see Guyton, 1981) to illustrate the priming effect. The first principle of neural transmission is that activation of a particular cell is an "all or none" (binary) proposition. Receptor sites on the cell (category) are stimulated by neural transmitters (primes) 23 to a certain minimum threshold point. Stimulation (priming) beyond the threshold level has no effect on the activation of the cell. The cell (category) either "fires" (becomes activated) or it does not. Once an action potential (activation of a category) is achieved, it slowly dissipates over time. The only advantage of hyperstimulation is to slow the rate at which this dissipation occurs. Based on assumptions inherent in the three theoretical models (i.e., storage bin. battery and synapse). Higgins, et al. (1985) formulated predictions regarding the relative importance of recency . and frequency of priming and the decay of the priming effect over time. They posited, for example, that the storage bin theory predicts a recently primed construct would predominate over constructs that were more frequently activated regardless of the length of delay between the prime and the stimulus presentation. Conversely, the battery model, which is based solely on level of activation, would generally favor the more frequently primed construct unless it had last been primed so long ago that its activation level decayed to a point below that of a more recently, but less frequently, primed construct. In any event. the battery model suggests that whichever construct possessed a higher level of activation (and was predominant) after a short delay period should also have a higher level of activation (and therefore be predominant) after a long delay between the prime and stimulus presentation. Finally, according to the authors, only the synapse model predicts a reversal between a more recently and a more frequently primed construct as a function of delay between final priming and stimulus presentation. Because recently and frequently primed constructs have a maximum action potential, they both begin 24 to dissipate immediately after their final prime, thereby giving the more recently primed construct the advantage after a short prime-to- stimulus delay. The more frequently activated construct, however, is hypothesized to dissipate at a slower rate, thereby giving it the advantage after a longer delay period. Despite the Higgins. et al. (1985) conclusion that their data were most consistent with predictions based on assumptions set forth in the synapse model of priming effect, Wyer and Srull (1986) contend their storage bin model could also "account for this finding if the probability of retrieving a particular concept from the attribute bin is fairly low" (p. 335). Despite their obvious differences, all three models (i.e., storage bin, battery cell, and synapse) resemble one another to the extent they each assume memory structures containing abstract representations of constructs (e.g., schemata) are in some way altered by exposure to a prior context. The need to assume the existence of such abstract constructs was brought into question, however, when Smith (1988) demonstrated that a computer simulation and an "exemplar-only memory model" was capable of reproducing several of the major features of social priming effects. Specifically, without imposing the existence of abstract cognitive constructs, Smith was able to reproduce previous findings that (a) category accessibility effects are influenced by the number of primes presented (Srull & Wyer, 1979); (b) behavioral primes have longer lasting effects when compared to trait-related word primes (Smith & Branscombe, 1987 ; Srull & Wyer, 1979); and (c) more recently presented primes exert a greater influence when there is a short delay between the prime and the stimulus, whereas more frequently presented primes have a greater 25 efiect when there is an extended prime-to-stimulus interval (Higgins et al., 1985). Although Smith's data imply that "category accessibility effects can no longer be taken as evidence for the use of abstract schemas or constructs in social perception" (p. 460), it is equally important to note that these data do not preclude the existence and use of such structures in the processing of social information. Further delineations of the priming effect. A number of investigations have identified conditions under which the previously described effects of social priming are mediated. Herr (1986; Herr, E Sherman & Fazio, 1983) and Martin (1986), for example, have conducted a series of investigations indicating the priming effect on social categorization may not be as straightforward as originally thought. Their research has demonstrated that under certain conditions, it is possible to predict that priming a trait category will result in judgments that are completely the reverse of those hypothesized by the priming effect. That is, rather than obtaining judgments skewed in the direction of the primed category (assimilation effects) certain characteristics of the prime. relative to the primed concept, result in judgments away from the prime (contrast effects). Like many findings in the area of cognitive priming, contrast effects were first reported in a non-social context. Kubovy (1977), for example, had subjects generate random digits after he varied the explicitness with which a particular digit was mentioned in the instructions. In the control condition, subjects were simply asked to "report the first digit that comes to mind." In two experimental variations of this condition, Kubovy altered the instructions by either 26 subtly or blatantly priming the subjects with the digit 1 (one). In the subtle priming condition, subjects were instructed to "report the first we digit that comes to mind" (italics added). In the second, and more blatant, priming condition subjects were told to "report the first digit that comes to mind, like ene" (italics added). Results indicated subjects in the control condition, who had received no priming at all, randomly chose the digit 1 about 2.2% of the time. As expected, responses from subjects exposed to the subtle prime were consistent with the traditional (assimilation) effects of priming. Approximately 18% of the subtly primed subjects "freely" chose the digit 1. In contrast, only 5.5% of the subjects exposed to the more blatant prime selected the digit 1. This curvilinear nature of the priming effect seemed to indicate that extreme priming of a construct could result in at least a reduced, if not totally reversed, priming effect. Herr, et al. (1983) likewise examined the effects of moderate and extreme priming on non-social categorization. In their study, subjects were primed with exemplars of (a) extremely small, (b) moderately small, (c) moderately large or (d) extremely large animals. The effects exposures to these exemplars had on the size estimate of an unreal, and therefore ambiguous animal, demonstrated that priming is capable of producing both assimilation (judgments in the direction of the prime) and contrast (judgments away from the prime) effects. Specifically, subjects exposed to extreme exemplars (either very small or very large animals) exhibited contrast effects. For example, when primed with extremer small animals, subjects' estimates regarding the size of an unreal animal were Egg than those given by subjects exposed to primes of either moderately small or moderately large __—______A 27 animals (contrast effects). On the other hand, subjects primed with moderate exemplars (moderately small or moderately large animals) tended to judge the size of an unreal animal to be closer to that of the primed category of animals (assimilation effects). In a study designed to extend these assimilation and contrast findings to social categorization, Herr (1986) unobtrusively exposed subjects to exemplars of famous personalities who were categorized as being either (a) extremely nonhostile, (b) moderately nonhostile, (c) moderately hostile, or (d) extremely hostile. Subjects' subsequent judgments of an ambiguously described individual supported the assimilation hypothesis that exposure to moderately hostile (e.g., Bobby Knight, Menachem Begin, etc.) or moderately nonhostile (e.g., Daniel Boone, Billie Jean King, etc.) primes would produce categorization judgments in the direction of that category. Conversely, priming with extreme hostile categories (e.g., Adolph Hitler, Charles Manson, etc.) resulted in lower hostility ratings of the ambiguous person than exposures to exemplars of either moderate category. Similar contrast effects were also observed in those subjects exposed to extremely nonhostile primes (e.g., Santa Claus, Shirley Temple, etc.). Martin (1986) has proposed and tested a theoretical model accounting for the seemingly contradictory assimilation and contrast effects of priming. According to Martin, factors other than the similarity or difference between the target and the priming context may be brought to bear when individuals use or disuse a prime. Simply put, assimilation or contrast will occur depending on whether or not the individual is prompted to use contextually activated (primed) 28 concepts when making impression formations. If an applicable concept, once it is primed, remains active beyond the original priming episode. it results in assimilation judgments toward the prime. Conversely, concepts that remain isolated to the priming episode are thought to activate distinct. alternative concepts which are, in turn, used to process and interpret information. In either case, exposure to the prime has caused the effect since it has elicited true changes in the way the subsequent information is interpreted. As Martin explains, "impressions may be a function not only of the concepts that are accessible to individuals but also of what individuals do with those concepts" (p. 503). In keeping with Martin's (1986) context x person interactionist perspective (cf. Endler, 1973; 1975; Silva 8r Weinberg, 1984) of priming effects, several other investigations have examined individual- specific factors that may influence category accessibility. Higgins, King and Mavin (1982), for example, hypothesized that subjects would be more inclined to use personally accessible constructs (as opposed to personally inaccessible constructs) when forming a social impression. To test their hypothesis, Higgins and his colleagues (Study 1) had subjects write down a maximum of 10 characteristics or traits found in (a) themselves, (b) two male friends and (c) two female friends. A trait was considered accessible to a particular subject if it was used to describe either himself/ herself and one of the friends. or at least three of the friends. Traits not meeting the criteria were considered inaccessible to the subject. Two weeks later, when subjects returned to participate in what was ostensibly an unrelated "person perception" study, they read an ambiguous behavioral 29 description of a person containing references to both accessible and inaccessible traits. After a lO-minute delay, subjects were asked to reproduce the behavioral description word-for-word and write, as completely as possible. their general impression of the person described in the essay. Results indicated that when forming an impression or reproducing the description, subjects were far less likely to omit references to traits that were personally accessible to them than traits to which they were inaccessible. In a related study, Higgins, et al. (1982: Study 2) exposed subjects to individually tailored descriptions containing six accessible and six inaccessible behavioral descriptions. Subjects were then "yoked" to one another in order to assure that a trait that was accessible to one subject was inaccessible to another. In addition to replicating their previous findings (Study 1). results of this second study revealed that accessibility to various trait constructs is quite idiosyncratic as demonstrated by only a 10% overlap between subjects in terms of their accessible constructs. The authors interpreted these findings in the following manner: ”If two perceivers have nonoverlapping accessible traits, their impressions and recollections of the same target person may have little in common . . . and there appears to be, in fact, relatively little overlap in people's accessible traits" (p. 44). Still another counter-intuitive, yet consistent, finding concerns the degree to which subjects are aware of the prime. Not only are reliable priming effects evident even when subjects are unable to recall the prime (Bargh & Pietromonaco, 1982), there is considerable evidence that suggests the effects are actually enhanced by the subliminal presentation of the primes (Bargh, 1982; Lombardi, Higgins 30 & Bargh, 1987). Specifically, Lombardi and her colleagues found that individuals who had the poorest recall of the prime tended to exhibit the largest priming effects. Qeggitive Schemag Notwithstanding Smith's (1988) findings that priming effects need not assume the presence of abstract knowledge structures, the most influential models set forth in an effort to explain the mechanisms underlying the priming effect (i.e., Wyer 8r Srull‘s (1981; 1986) storage bin model and Higgins. et al.'s (1985) energy transmission models) rely on the existence of these abstract memory structures. In each model, cognitive schemata (or closely related concepts) are activated by the priming event, thereby making them more accessible -- and more likely to be employed -- when encountering and processing information in the future. Because schemata are generally viewed as the cognitive mechanisms activated by the prime, it is necessary to understand the historical development and ongoing research into these underlying hypothetical constructs. Histerical overview of echeme theery. Although it is generally acknowledged that the neurologist H. Head (1918; 1926) is responsible for formulating the initial schema hypothesis, it is Bartlett's (1932) elaboration and extension that has inspired most modern schema theories (Brewer & Nakamura, 1984). According to Bartlett, schemata are unconscious, organized, abstracted knowledge structures involved in actively encoding, storing and retrieving information. Despite schema theory's eventual impact on cognitive psychology and its early acceptance in England, Bartlett's contributions went virtually unnoticed for nearly half a century. In W 31 large part. this lack of recognition was due to the fact that Bartlett's theory had its origins in the Continental philosophical tradition (e. g., Immanuel Kant, etc.) which assumes the existence of unconscious, complex mental structures of knowledge.2 Unlike Bartlett's homeland of England, where the Continental philosophy exerted considerable influence on the development of psychological theory, mainstream American psychology was, at the time, totally committed to the theoretical descendants of the British Empiricists (e.g., George Berkeley, Thomas Hobbs, David Hume, John Locke, James Mill. John Stuart Mill, etc.). The atomistic. mechanistic, associationistic and passive views of learning and memory such as those espoused by popular stimulus—response theorists (e.g., Ivan Pavlov, John B. Watson, E. L. Thorndike and B. F. Skinner, etc.) left little room for the serious consideration of vague and "soft—headed" theories dealing with the complex, unconscious. organized, abstracted knowledge structures Bartlett proposed. In fact, the neo-Empiricist tradition so dominated American psychological thought for the first 70 years of the twentieth century that by the time of his death in 1969, even Bartlett's disciples (e.g., Broadbent, 1970; Oldfied, 1972; Zangwill, 1972, etc.) had resolved that schema theory had died with him (Brewer & Nakamura, 1984). Two necessary antecedents for the resurrection of schema theory occurred during the 19605 and 70s. First, and foremost, there was a major paradigm shift "away from stimulus—response units to describe perception and action toward a recognition that human beings bring meaning and organization into almost every new encounter" (Singer 81 Salovey, 1991; pp. 33-34). Specifically, psychologists who were 32 originally attracted to the tough-mindedness of the British Empiricist position, were finally, as Brewer and Nakamura (1984) put it, "dragged 'kicking and screaming' by the brute facts of nature to the Continental position" (p. 129). Second, it became easier to understand (and explain via analogies) the basic properties of schemata when technological advances allowed for the building of computer systems that "employed schema-like entities [computer programs] as a fundamental part of their operations" (Rumelhart, 1984; p. 162). Finally, by 1975, the philosophical and technological conditions existed which were compatible with the rebirth of the schema construct. In that year, papers employing variations of the schema theory were published in such diverse content areas as artificial intelligence (Minsky, 1975), cognitive psychology (Rumelhart, 1975) and motor performance (Schmidt, 1975). In addition. a number of schema-related concepts gained popularity at this time. Examples of these "schema-compatible concepts" (Taylor & Crocker. 1981) include "frames" (Minsky, 1975), "prototypes" (Cantor & Mischel, 1977), and "scripts" (Abelson, 1975). Scheme meog. Despite lacking a fixed definition for the term "schema," theories employing the schema concept have been "used to guide or explain a considerable portion of current research in human memory" (Alba & Hasher, 1983; p. 203). These theories generally assume that knowledge, whether it pertains to an inanimate object (e.g., chair. desk, lamp, etc.), an event (e.g., washing clothes, driving a car, etc.). a social situation (e.g., a cocktail party; a college classroom, etc.) or a social entity (e.g, Richard Nixon, my friend David, me. etc.). is abstracted and organized into modular structures of related 33 information called schemata. Once they are constructed on the basis of prior experience, these organized knowledge structures serve as idiosyncratic cognitive frameworks by which sensory input is actively interpreted to form a meaningful reality. The way in which these knowledge structures are capable of abstracting previous experiences, interpreting incoming information and initiating novel responses can be illustrated by examining Schmidt's (1975) motor schema theory. Schmidt's theory attempts to explain -- using the schema concept -— how individuals learn and control new motor skills. According to the theory, any time a motor skill of any type is performed, four specific types of information are abstracted from the movement experience: (a) imeg cengitien -- environmental and body conditions at the time the movement was initiated, (b) respense speeificetiene -— the task demands in terms of speed, force, etc., (c) sensogz eenseguenees -— sensory input from a variety of sources, both during and after the movement, and (d) respenee outcom -- basically, knowledge of results. This abstracted information is called upon whenever the individual is faced with performing a novel motor skill. For example, when throwing a javelin for the first time, a performer may demonstrate surprising ability by drawing on previously abstracted movement experiences stored in memory. Despite having no direct experience throwing a javelin, he or she has a general motor schema for the required "overhand throw." Key elements involving the initial conditions, task specifications, sensations and response outcomes of previous (similar) motor experiences such as hitting an overhead tennis serve or throwing a 34 baseball, softball or football have all been stored in memory and serve as a guide when performing the novel motor task. Schemata, according to Singer and Salovey (1991) "allow the perceiver to identify stimuli quickly, cluster them into manageable units, fill in missing information, and select a strateg for obtaining further information in order to solve a problem or reach a goal" (p. 35). The essential role schema play in the processing of new information is evidenced by the fact they have been described as no less than "the basis of the selectivity that is operative in information processing" (Markus & Smith, 1981; p. 240) and "the fundamental elements upon which all information processing depends" (Rumelhart, 1984; p. 162). Rumelhart (1984) offers several analogies in an attempt to describe the nature and operation of schemata. His first analog is that of a theatrical play where schemata function as "scripts" containing prototypes of characters, roles, motives, actions, and interactions. These event or situation schemata serve as a general framework that enables us to quickly assemble otherwise fragmentary and uninterpretable stimuli into a coherent and meaningful perception of reality. Although the details of this script can change considerably, the general nature of the "performance" is identified as being the same. An example of an event schema (script) we all have is "buying a car." A great deal of variation can take place regarding the specifics of this schema. The variables in our script can take on many (but not all) values. The schema provides information about the event by setting legitimate value ranges within which this event must take place. For example, the buyer or seller of the car may be a man or woman, young W; ’ ” 35 or old, large or small, fat or thin, intelligent or stupid but not a grizzly bear. The car may be old or new, large or small, expensive or inexpensive but not made of soap. The transaction may take place publicly at an automobile showroom, in a restaurant, or in a private home but probably not in the midst of a funeral procession. This framework is effective in giving us a quick, usable notion of what the event "buying a car" usually entails. A second analog proposes that an individual's schemata, when taken as a whole, function much like a personal theory of reality. Schemata, like all useful scientific theories, are constantly being evaluated on the basis of their ability to adequately represent, and thereby explain, the real world. When a particular schema (theory) is activated in an attempt to explain incoming information (data), something analogous to a "goodness-of-fit test" is calculated to determine the degree to which the schemata (theory) assists in the comprehension (explanation) of the information (data). If the information (data) generally "fits," the schema (hypothesis) is accepted. To the extent that the activated schemata (theory) fails to account for all the information (data), it can either be modified or totally rejected in favor of a better, more explanatory model of reality. Another analog is drawn to computer program procedures. First, just as computers are capable of discerning the degree of fit between a given observation and a previously assigned value, schemata allow us to classify information based on the degree to which it matches a previously specified prototype. Second, computers often employ a number of "sub-routines" to perform a complex task. Similarly, complex schematic representations -- such as those that define the W} ” 36 recognition of a human face -- often exist at the top of a procedural hierarchy of sub-schemata (e.g., human eye, human mouth, etc.) that examine and determine whether there is an acceptable degree of fit between the observation and previously formed schemata. The sub- schema for a human eye, may in turn, have several sub-schemata of its own which look for acceptable variations in number (two, not seven), color (blue, brown, etc., but not orange), shape (oval, not square), size (not six inches in diameter) and spatial relationships (on the front upper third of the face, not on the sides of the head). Similar to the operation of a computer program's sub-routines, these individual judgments are sent back to higher level schemata which determines whether the total observation is within the "acceptable limits" imposed by the schema. These analogies tend to emphasize the common belief that schemata generally process information in a theory-driven or "top- down" fashion (e.g., Bransford & Johnson, 1972; Singer 8: Salovey, 1991). Top-down processing basically assumes that incoming sensory data are assimilated into an existing knowledge structure. Singer and Salovey offer the following clever illustration of how an activated (instantiated) schema can drive the interpretation of ambiguous data: Suppose one of us shows you a photograph of a man, standing in his shorts, holding a glass filled with a yellowish liquid. If we then tell you that this is a photograph of someone standing by the pool on a hot day, instantiating your schema about poolside behavior, you will conclude that the yellow liquid is lemonade and the shorts are really a bathing suit. On the other hand. suppose we tell you this is a photo of a man with a bladder infection at a 37 recent visit to the urologist. You now conclude that he is really wearing boxer shorts. And what do you no_w think the glass of yellow liquid is? (p. 37). Rumelhart (1984), however, also stresses the less conventional view that schemata are equally capable of processing information in a "bottom-up" data- or event—driven manner. By analyzing the interpretations people make after being presented with fragmentary information, Rumelhart (1981) uses the following example to illustrate how comprehension is the result of both data-driven theory construction m conceptually-driven theory testing: Business had been slow since the oil crisis. Nobody seemed to want anything really elegant anymore. Suddenly the door opened and a well dressed man entered the showroom floor. John put on his friendliest and most sincere expression and walked toward the man (p. 171). Although very little factual information is provided in the preceding paragraph, most people conclude that John is a automobile salesman specializing in the sale of large, American-made, luxury automobiles. The well dressed man is a potential customer, the entire scene is taking place in an automobile showroom and John is approaching the man with the hope of selling him a new car. Rumelhart (1981) summarizes the way this fragmentary information is generally interpreted as follows: A plausible schema doesn't simply "suggest itself," but instead. is a direct function of an initial observation. A "business" schema is therefore activated by reading the first sentence. The exact nature of the business is, at this point, still unknown but it must be related, in some way, to the oil industry. Once 38 this bottom-up activation of the business schema has taken place, the schema would, in the traditional top-down fashion, generate a list of plausible businesses by conducting a goodness-of—fit test on this 011— related criterion. Automobile-related businesses are prime candidates. This business schema also tends to activate a "buy" and/ or "sell" schema that places automobile and gasoline sales at the top of the list. The introduction of the second sentence effectively eliminates the gasoline hypothesis leaving only the automobile sales schema in tact. The remaining sentences are examined for the degree to which they support or fail to support the automobile sales hypothesis. Because each subsequent sentence supports and reinforces the validity of this hypothesis, it is eventually accepted. In summary, Rumelhart (1984: Rumelhart & Ortony, 1977) suggests that in addition to their ability to engage in both top-down and bottom-up information processing, schemata have six general characteristics. Specifically, schemata: (a) have variables: (b) can embed, one within another; (c) represent knowledge at all levels of abstraction; (d) represent knowledge rather than definitions; (e) are active processes: and (f) are recognition devices whose processing is aimed at the evaluation of the goodness—of—fit to the data being processed (Rumelhart, 1984; p. 169). Self—Schemata. Many different types of cognitive schemata are required to process the vast amount of social and non-social information human beings encounter. Taylor and Crocker (1981) have identified several subcategories of social schemata on the basis of their content, such as those used to process and categorize information pertaining to (a) people (person schemata); (b) appropriate norms and 39 behaviors of broadly defined groups of individuals (role schemata); or (c) social situations or events (event schemata). In addition, much of the information humans need to process is information used to perceive, remember and make inferences about themselves (self- schemata). Although theorists have conceptually and operationally defined these unique instances of person schemata in a variety of ways, an examination of Table 2 reveals that self-schemata are typically viewed as organized general knowledge structures containing information about the self. As is the case with social schemata as a whole, self-schemata enable the individual to form an abstracted or generalized version of a social entity, which is then used as a perceptual filter through which incoming information is subsequently processed. One important aspect of self-schemata that makes them unlike other types of social and nonsocial schemata, however, is the fact that self-schemata are the perceptual filters for information concerning the most important of social entities -- the "self." Some theorists, in fact, have gone so far as to define self-concept as nothing more than "a system of self-schema " (Markus 8: Smith, 1981, p. 242; Markus & Sentis, 1982, p.45) or "a complex, person-specific, central, attitudinal schema" (Greenwald & Patkanis, 1984, p. 130). Such views imply that several different self-schemata may be included within an individual's concept of his or her "self." For example, a number of equally plausible self-schemata: "introvert," "musician," "athlete," "honors student," etc. may all coexist as part of a particular individual's view of himself or herself. The specific self-schema that becomes activated, and therefore used as a framework for the interpretation of subsequent stimuli, is simply a function of whichever schema is most Table 2. 40 Conceptual Definitions of Self-Schema. Auther Qoneepmfl Definitien Markus (1977) Markus 6: Smith (1981) Derry & Kuiper (1 98 l) Cantor, Mischel & Schwartz ( l 982) Fiske & Taylor (1984) Klein & Kihlstrom (l 986) Cognitive generalizations about the self, derived from past expectancies, that organize and guide the processing of self-related information contained in an individual's social experience. Same as Markus (1977), noting also that self-schemas provide an anchor or frame of reference for judgments and evaluations of others. A hierarchically organized body of knowledge stored in long-term memory, a list of general and specific terms characteristic of the individual that have been derived from a lifetime of experience with personal data. A set of beliefs about the self through which interpretations of other people's behaviors and social interactions are filtered. General information about one's own psycholog makes up a complex, easily accessible verbal self-concept that guides information processing about the self. Superior recall for adjectives when a self—relevant information-processing set is adopted. Adapted from Singer 8r Salovey (1991). 4 1 salient at that particular time (Markus & Smith, 1981). Several factors such as motivations. relevancy, frequency of activation, or recency of activation may contribute to which, among the several possible self-schemata available, is most salient at a particular time (Fiske & Taylor, 1984; Higgins, et al., 1985). Self-schemata were initially viewed as theoretical constructs or epiphenomena which were used primarily as post hoc explanations of observed behavior. Markus (1977), however, expanded this rather limited application by specifying and empirically testing hypothesized functions of self-schema. Markus posited that it is possible to identify, via self-report measures, individuals who either possess (schematics) or do not possess (aschematics), a self-schema for a particular behavioral domain. Once identified, these individuals can be subjected to a variety of cognitive tasks related to the domain. Markus' underlying assumption is that observed difierences in the way schematics and aschematics process domain—related information is attributable, in part, to the presence or absence of a self-schema (Markus & Sentis, 1982). In what has since become a classic paradigm for examining the effects of self-schemata on information processing, Markus (1977 ; Study 1) had 101 female undergraduates rate themselves on semantic differential scales describing a variety of behavioral domains. In addition, subjects were asked to "rate the importance of each semantic dimension to their self-description" (p. 66). On the basis of these preliminary data, Markus was able to assign 16 subjects to each of three experimental groups: (a) independent schematics - subjects rating themselves as highly independent and perceiving the 42 dependence/independence dimension as being important to their self-description; (b) dependent echemetiee - subjects rating themselves as highly dependent and perceiving the dependence/independence dimension as being important to their self-description; and (c) eeehemaflce — subjects with self—ratings in the middle range of the dependence/independence dimension and importance ratings suggesting this dimension was not central to their self—description. Three to four weeks after the initial assessment, subjects were briefly exposed to words that were either independence—related, dependence-related or creativity-related (control). Subjects were instructed to indicate whether or not the word was descriptive of them by pushing either a "me" or a "not me" button. Not only was there the expected difference between the groups in the absolute number of "me" and "not me" self—classifications (i.e., independent schematics indicating "me" to more independence- related words and dependent schematics indicating "me" to more dependence-related words) but response latencies indicated that subjects responded faster to words that were consistent with their self-schema. Specifically, independent schematics responded faster to independence-related words; dependent schematics responded faster to dependence-related words and aschematics responded with approximately equal speed to both independence- and dependence- related words. These findings suggest that having an existing schema on a particular behavioral domain enables the schematic to process schema-related information in a somewhat different (and faster) manner that non—schematics. 43 To test the hypothesis that possessing a self—schema on a behavioral dimension makes an individual resistant to counter- schematic information, Markus (197 7 ; Study 2) arranged for the same subjects to return to the lab after a three week interval. In an ostensibly unrelated task, subjects completed a fictitious suggestibility measure and were given bogus feedback that was inconsistent with their previously identified self-schemata (i.e., independent schematics were told they were highly dependent and suggestible; dependent schematics were told they were highly independent and not at all suggestible; aschematics were randomly assigned to one of the two feedback groups). After receiving the schema-inconsistent feedback, all subjects again performed the "me"/"not me" latency response task described in Study 1 . Results indicated that aschematics were more likely than either independent or dependent schematics to: (a) agree with the bogus feedback; (b) agree to take the test again; and (0) exhibit greater inconsistency in their "me"/"not me" responses. In her general discussion of these findings, Markus concluded that the results of these two studies "provide converging evidence for the concept of self-schemata, or cognitive generalizations about the self, which organize, summarize and explain behavior along a particular dimension." (p. 75) In addition to replicating Markus' (1977) initial findings, Sentis and Markus (1979; cited in Markus 8r Sentis, 1982) tested the hypothesis that schematics differ from aschematics not only in the way they process stimuli, but also in the way they remember self-relevant information. After taking part in the "me/ not me" judgment task used by Markus, subjects were exposed to a series of adjectives. Some of 44 these adjectives had been part of the previous "me/not me" task and others had not. Subjects were asked to respond by pressing a button labeled "OLD" if the adjective had been used in the previous judgment task and a button labeled "NEW" if they did not remember being previously exposed to the adjective. As hypothesized, schematics showed superior recognition for schema-related information. Specifically, schematics responded faster and more accurately than aschematics when presented with schema-relevant adjectives. A number of subsequent investigations have extended these findings by hypothesizing and empirically testing the information processing functions of self-schemata in a variety of behavioral domains. Similar schemata-related results have been demonstrated in the areas of creativity, extraversion, dominance, body weight, sex roles and social sensitivity (Markus & Sentis, 1982). Although it is frequently implied that self-schemata can exert influences on behavior in the same way they have been shown to influence perception, inference and memory, few empirical investigations have examined the link between self-schema and behavior. One exception is a series of studies by Kendzierski (1988; 1990) which focuses on behavior by individuals classified as having either (a) a self-schema for exercising (exerciser schematics); (b) a self-schema for not exercising (nonexerciser schematics); or (c) a self- schema which favored neither exercising nor non-exercising (aschematics). Each subject rated 12 pairs of behavioral alternatives (one of which reflected a pro-exercise orientation, the other alternative did not) on a 100-point scale indicating the likelihood they would behave in the way described. Results indicated that exerciser 45 schematics thought they would be more likely to choose the exercise- oriented alternative than did either aschematics or nonexerciser schematics. In addition, aschematics were more likely to predict they would choose the exercise-oriented alternative than the nonexerciser schematics. In summarizing the findings of these empirical investigations, Markus and Sentis (1982) state that individuals with self-schemata in particular domains generally: "(1) process domain-related information with relative ease and certainty: (2) are consistent in their responses; (3) have relatively better recognition and recall memory for information relevant to this domain; and (4) can predict future behavior in the domain" (p. 62). Self-Effieacy Contrary to the lack of research examining the relationship between the accessibility (priming) of various social schemata and motor performance, another cognitive factor -- self-efficacy -- has long been a major focus of research by sport psychologists. According to Feltz (1992) more than two dozen studies have been published on the topic of self-efficacy in sport and physical activity in the past 15 years. Not surprisingly, situation—specific self-confidence is, in fact, one of the most frequently cited psychological variables thought to affect motor performance (e.g., Feltz, 1988b; McAuley & Gill, 1983; Weinberg, Gould & Jackson, 1979). In sport contexts, coaches, athletes and fans often consider a performer's confidence level to be a major determinant of success and failure. Although, in its everyday use, the term "self-confidence" typically refers to one's perceived ability to accomplish a particular performance objective (e.g., pass a 46 final math examination, perform a gmnastic routine, etc.). Bandura (1977) has suggested that the use of the term self-confidence be reserved to describe the global personality trait that depicts an individual’s overall level of performance optimism. Perceived self- efficacy, on the other hand, has been defined by Bandura (1986) as: People's judgments of their capabilities to organize and execute courses of action required to attain designated types of performances. It is concerned not with the skills one has but with judgments of what one can do with whatever skills one possesses [and as such becomes] a significant determinant of performance that operates partially independently of underlying skills." (p. 391) Based on this definition, self-efficacy can be thought to be something akin to a task- and/ or situation-specific form of confidence (Feltz, 1988b). Research lends support to Bandura's (1977; 1986) claim that a global or general measure of self-confidence is not as useful as situation-specific self-efficacy judgments in predicting subsequent performance. McAuley and Gill (1983), for example, compared female collegiate gmnasts' confidence in their general physical ability and their situation-specific self-efficacy by correlating both measures with their subsequent performance during an intercollegiate competition. They found that although general physical ability confidence was not a significant predictor of skilled performance, the gmnasts' task- specific self-efficacy judgments were highly correlated with actual performance scores. 47 According to Bandura (1977, 1986) an individual's efficacy expectations are derived from four major sources of self—referent information. The first, and generally most powerful source of efficacy expectations result from the perception of previeps perfermapee attgnments. Simply put, a personal history of perceived successes in similar situations or on similar tasks will tend to raise efficacy assessments while a personal history of perceived failures on these tasks will tend to lower efficacy appraisals. It should be noted that it is the subjective feeling of success or failure in related situations -- not objective success or failure itself -— that drives the formation of efficacy judgments. A second. and somewhat related source of information regarding an individual's capabilities is based on observing or visualizing the results of others' (individuals perceived to be similar in some way to the observer) attempts to perform the task in question. Such vieeg'ous experience rarely provides the powerful and accurate self-referent information available via previous personal performance but in the absence of such first—hand experience, information derived from the knowledge of others' performances can be used to accurately infer self-efficacy judgments. Verbal persuasien or encouragement, a technique frequently employed by coaches and teachers, can provide a third major source of self—referent information on which self-efficacy appraisals are made. In general, self-efficacy expectations resulting from persuasive techniques are only effective to the extent that the performer is given what he or she perceives to be relevant and realistic information regarding his or her ability to perform a specific task. Finally, Bandura suggests that the assessment ef phfiielegieel states can provide valuable information upon which to base individual 48 efficacy expectations. For example, because excessive physiological arousal is frequently associated with -- and therefore useful in predicting -- a poor performance on skilled tasks, individuals can make capability judgments, in part, on the basis of their current physiological arousal level. Arousal is not, however, the only physiological state that can provide such cues to performance. Bandura notes that other physical states such as fatigue, windedness, aches, and pains can all provide valuable information regarding an individual's performance of a particular task. Feltz (1992; 1988b), in her reviews of the self-efficacy literature pertaining to sport and physical activity, summarizes research examining each of the four major sources of information used in the formation of efficacy expectations (i.e., previous performance attainments, vicarious learning, persuasion and assessment of physiological states). In regard to prior performance accomplishments, she concluded that considerable empirical evidence exists supporting Bandura's (1977 ; 1986) contention that performance-based information is not only associated with subsequent self-efficacy judgments (e.g., Feltz, 1982; Feltz, Landers & Raeder, 1979; Hogan & Santomier, 1984; McAuley, 1985; Weinberg, et al., 1979) but exerts more influence on self-efficacy judgments than any of the other three sources of information (Deshamais, Bouillon & Godin, 1986; Feltz, et al., 1979; Feltz 8r Mugno, 1983; McAuley, 1985; Weinberg, Sinardi & Jackson, 1982). Similarly, empirical support was found for the notion that vicarious experiences can likewise (albeit at a somewhat weaker level) influence efficacy judgments. Performance- related information gleaned by (a) being exposed to the results of a 49 model's attempt at the task (Corbin, Laurie, Gruger 8r Smiley, 1984; Gould 8r Weiss, 1981), (b) visually imaging the outcome of a performance (Feltz, Marcotullio 8r Fitzgerald, 1985), or (c) learning more about a competitor's ability (Weinberg, et al, 1979; Weinberg, Gould, Yukelson & Jackson, 198 l; Weinberg, Yukelson 8: J ackson, 1980) have all been shown to impact on efficacy judgments. Somewhat surprising, given the emphasis placed on the role of verbal exhortation and physiological awareness in sport and physical activity, is the relatively small number of studies examining the extent to which information from the two remaining sources -- verbal persuasion and assessment of physiological states -- is used in the formation of efficacy expectations. One indication of the dearth of research into verbal persuasion as a source of efficacy judgments in the motor performance area is that in her extensive reviews of the literature, Feltz (1992; 1986b) cites only four published self-efiicacy studies falling under the rubric of persuasive techniques. In fact, even those investigations did not directly address what is commonly thought to be the classic case of verbal persuasion -- performance— related encouragement from others. Instead, persuasion in these studies has been broadly defined as positive sefl-talk (Weinberg, 1986; Wilkes 8r Summers, 1984) imagery (Feltz & Riessinger, 1990; Wilkes & Summers, 1984) and cognitive restructuring of task-related arousal (Y an Lan 8r Gill, 1984). Unfortunately, even these peripheral investigations provide little in the way of unequivocal results concerning the effects of persuasive techniques on self-efficacy judgments (Feltz, 1992). 50 It is Feltz's own line of research (Feltz, 1982: Feltz & Albrecht, 1986; Feltz & Mugno, 1983) examining self-efficacy judgments for a high-avoidance motor task (modified back diving) that has provided the most insight into the manner in which physiological states (i.e., arousal) affect efficacy expectations in a motor performance situation. Using path analytic techniques to test Bandura's (1977) hypothesis that a reciprocal relationship exists between physiological arousal and self-efficacy, Feltz (1982) found that the heart rate —-) self-efficacy paths "were not at all useful in determining efficacy expectations for diving performance" (p. 771). In addition, only the initial (prior to performance) self-efficacy —> heart rate path was found to be significant. Surprisingly, however, the relationship was in the opposite direction (positive) of that predicted by Bandura. Although these findings were successfully replicated in two subsequent investigations (Feltz & Albrecht, 1986; Feltz & Mugno, 1983) both of these studies found evidence that it is the individual's pgeeiled level of autonomic arousal that is more closely associated with efficacy expectations than actual physiological arousal (i.e., heart rate). The extent to which Bandura's more recent writings (Bandura, 1986) have taken these findings into account is evidenced by the fact that the source of efficacy expectations originally referred to as "emotional arousal" (Bandura, 1977) now emphasizes the "eognip've processing of emotional reactivity" (Bandura, 1986: p. 406, italics added). Although considerable attention has been given to Bandura's (1977 , 1986) four major sources of efficacy information, sometimes overlooked has been his qualification that none of these sources -- either individually or in combination -- is inherently capable of 5 1 generating efficacy expectations. Rather, it is the cognitive processing of the information from each of these sources that conveys an efficacy expectation. As Bandura (1986) puts it: The cognitive processing of efficacy information involves two separable functions: The first concerns the types of information people attend to and use as indicators of personal efficacy. . . The second concerns the combination rules or heuristics they employ for weighting and integrating efficacy information from different sources in forming their self-efficacy judgments. (p. 401) It is, therefore, Bandura's contention that efi‘icacy expectations result from an individual W attending, weighting and integrating a portion of all available information relevant to the likelihood of her or his successful performance on a particular task. Furthermore, this emphasis on selective information processing implies that any factor capable of making either positive or negative self-referent cognitions more accessible and salient during information processing (e.g., cognitive priming of positive or negative self-schemata) may exert considerable influence on subsequent self-efficacy judgments (and thereby, indirectly affect performance). M i M i Eff f M A considerable amount of empirical evidence can be cited supporting the contention that mood states, under certain conditions, can influence -— or be influenced by -- self-schemata, self-efficacy and performance. Unfortunately, a perusal of this literature is somewhat disconcerting in that mood appears to have the potential to be both a cause and / or an effect for nearly any cognitive variable of interest. 52 The relationship between meod and self-sehemate. As generalized knowledge structures containing information about the self, self-schemata have, as their basis, an individual's past experiences. The fact that naturally occurring and experimentally induced mood states can alter the relative accessibility of memory structures has been demonstrated in a number of studies (e.g., Bower, 1981; Clark & Teasdale, 1982; Clark & Teasdale. 1985; Forgas, Bower & Krantz, 1984; Isen, Shalker, Clark & Karp, 1978: Natale & Hantas, 1982; Teasdale & Fogarty, 1979). In one study examining the effect mood has on recall of past events, Isen, et al. (1978; Study 1) induced a good mood by having a confederate give people at a shopping mall a small gift (a notepad or a nail clipper). In an ostensibly unrelated event, another confederate later approached the individual and asked him or her to complete a "consumer survey." Subjects randomly assigned to the experimental condition (those receiving the free gift) gave higher ratings of the performance and service records of products they owned than subjects in the control (no gift) condition. In another investigation of mood effects on memory, Bower (1981) had subjects record, in detail, all positive and negative emotional events that happened to them over the course of a week. In addition to recording the time, place, participants and overall gist of the event, the subject rated each event on a lO-point scale from "pleasant" to "unpleasant." At the end of the week, the subjects were induced into either a happy or sad mood via hypnotic suggestion and were asked to recall as many diary entries as possible. Subjects put into a pleasant mood were more likely to recall pleasant emotional 53 experiences from the previous week whereas those in a negative mood recalled more negative emotional events. In addition, when asked to rate the current emotional intensity of each event, subjects in a good mood tended to rate the event as being even more pleasant than they had at the time it occurred. Conversely, subjects in a sad mood rated the emotional intensity of their recalled experiences to be even more unpleasant than they had originally indicated. In summarizing the results of the myriad experimental investigations into the effects of mood on memory, Clark and Teasdale (1985) conclude: Memories of positive hedonic tone are more accessible in experimentally induced elated moods than in experimentally induced depressed mood. Conversely, memories of negative hedonic tone tend to be more accessible in induced depressed mood than in induced elated mood (p. 1595). Similarly, after observing that individuals clinically diagnosed as depressives tended to selectively focus on past and present negative information about themselves, Beck (1967, 1976) proposed that naturally occurring mood states may be associated with distortions in the processing of information about the self. The depressed patient, according to Beck constructs a "cognitive triad" consisting of (a) a negative view of the world, (b) a negative self-concept, and (c) a negative appraisal of his or her future. Subsequent support for this contention has been provided by a number of investigations (e.g., Breslow, Kocsis & Belkin, 1981; Clark & Teasdale, 1982: Loyd 8r Lishman, 197 5) which have found that "natural fluctuations in depressed mood within patients have effects on the accessibility of 54 positive and negative information similar to those observed in studies of induced mood states" (Clark & Teasdale, 1985; p. 1595). Given the extent of empirical and anecdotal evidence suggesting that the recall of positive or negative memories can be made more salient by mood, it is conceivable that mood-congruent generic knowledge structures based on past experiences (self-schemata) may be activated by the induction of a positive or negative mood. Th rel tion hi between moo lf- ffi c . As a means of introducing their research examining the impact of happy and sad moods on efficacy judgments, Kavanagh and Bower (1985) succinctly frame the question of interest as follows: "Do people feel more competent when they are happy than when they are sad?" (p. 507). They then provide the reader with a preliminary answer based solely on intuition and experience: "Certainly when we consider our own experience, we would answer affirmatively. When we are sufficiently elated, we feel able to achieve our highest ambitions. On the other hand, when we are feeling low, failure seems inevitable" (p. 507). Although the notion that moods can directly impact on self-efficacy is intuitively appealing, the few empirical studies conducted in this area have produced equivocal results. An investigation by Wright and Mischel (1982), for example. supports the predicted relationship between mood and efficacy expectations. In the study, subjects were induced into positive, negative or neutral moods by having them recall highly positive, negative or neutral personal experiences. Once induced with the desired mood, the subjects were given bogus feedback related to their performance on a complex perceptual task. As hypothesized, high self-efficacy judgments were obtained when 55 success feedback was given to subjects possessing a positive or neutral mood. In contrast, low self-efficacy judgments resulted when failure feedback was given to subjects in a negative or neutral mood. Even more interesting were the interactions obtained when the mood and feedback were incongruent. Subjects in positive moods receiving failure feedback, tended to overestimate their future performance while those in negative moods receiving success feedback underestimated the probability of future success. Kavanagh and Bower (1985) similarly found support for the hypothesis of mood influences on self-efficacy. While in a hypnotic trance, all subjects were placed, at different times, into a happy, sad and neutral mood by having them recall happy, sad and neutral romantic experiences. Once in the desired mood, subjects responded to self-efficacy items on scales pertaining to (a) romantic situations, (b) social skills and assertiveness, and (0) academic, athletic and other physical activities. When the subjects were in negative moods, they produced the lowest efiicacy ratings on all scales. On the other hand, the highest efficacy ratings for all scales were obtained when subjects were in a positive mood. Finally, as predicted, when in neutral moods, subjects scored between the two extremes on all three scales. Interestingly, recalling a happy, sad or neutral romantic experiences seemed to influence not only efficacy judgments in a closely related (romantic) context, but also generalized to efficacy judgments in less related areas of social, academic, athletic and general physical activity. In contrast to investigations supporting the influence mood exerts on self-efficacy, Stanley and Maddux (1986; Experiment 2) not only failed to demonstrate that moods influence self-efficacy. but actually 56 found evidence that this relationship was reversed —- self-efficacy has an effect on mood (Experiment 1). In order to examine self-efficacy effects on mood, Stanley and Maddux informed subjects they would be performing a social interaction task (meet another student, talk with him, make him feel comfortable, find a common interest and get him to like them). Prior to the task, however, subjects completed a questionnaire and were given bogus feedback regarding their social skills. The bogus feedback indicated the subject either possessed (a) below average (low self-efficacy expectancy) or (b) above average (high self-efficacy expectancy) social skills. Results of the study indicated that self-efficacy expectancy manipulation had a significant effect on subject mood state. Specifically, subjects in the low self-efficacy expectancy condition (those told they had below average social skills) reported having a more depressed mood than those in the high self- efficacy expectancy condition (those told they had above average social skills). In the second part of their investigation (Stanley & Maddux, 1986: Experiment 2) the authors examined the effect mood has on self-efficacy. Using a mood induction procedure developed by Velten (1968), which involves reading 60 depression (e.g., "I want to go to sleep and never wake up") or 60 elation (e.g., "Things will be better and better today") self-referent mood statements, subjects were placed in either an elated or depressed mood state. Once this was accomplished, subjects completed self—efficacy judgments regarding the same social interaction task used in Experiment 1. In contrast to the findings of Wright and Mischel (1982) and Kavanagh and Bower (1985), the investigators reported that the Velten procedure of mood 57 induction had no effect on subjects' self-efficacy expectancy ratings for the social interaction task or for social situations in general. In another study that failed to clearly demonstrate the mediating effects of mood on self-efficacy (and the only study using a motor performance task), Kavanagh and Hausfeld (1986) manipulated subjects' mood by having them listen to happy, neutral (Experiment 1) and sad (Experiment 2) audiotapes. Once the desired mood had been attained, subjects rated their degree of confidence that they could (a) exert 18 different levels of force on a handgrip dynamometer (Experiment 1); and (b) do 18 levels of push-ups (Experiment 2). Although a manipulation check revealed that listening to the audiotapes significantly altered the subj ects' mood, there was no difference in handgrip efficacy ratings in the three mood conditions. On the other hand, mood did have an effect on the subjects' push-up efficacy judgments (Experiment 2). The results of the aforementioned investigations into the relationship between mood states and self-efficacy leaves many unanswered questions. Despite its intuitive appeal, the influences mood states have on self-efficacy has yet to be adequately demonstrated. Primin f lf-Rfrn ni n limping ef self-sehemata. Although the priming of generalized abstracted knowledge structures (schemata) has a strong theoretical and empirical basis (Johnson 8r Dark, 1986), much of this research has focused on the accessibility of social categories not directly related to the "self." The intent of these studies has generally been to examine the role category accessibility plays in the processing of 58 information relative to other social entities (Higgins, et al., 1977; Higgins, et al., 1982; Higgins, et al., 1985; Wyer 8: Srull, 1981; 1986). Somewhat more germane to the present investigation is research conducted by Baldwin and his colleagues (Baldwin, Carrell & Lopez, 1990: Baldwin & Holmes, 1987) examining the impact of priming cognitive representations of significant others on the experience of the self. In the Baldwin and Holmes study, for example, female subjects were primed with (presumably) permissive and non- permissive social attitudes by having them visualize the faces of either their parents (non-permissive) or two friends from school (permissive). Under the guise of a separate study, subjects were later asked to rate the enjoyableness of several written passages. The specific passage of interest contained a description of a sexual encounter which depicted a rather permissive attitude toward sexuality. Results supported the prediction that the visualization of the significant other (permissive vs. non-permissive) would serve to prime either a permissive or non-permissive social attitude and thus influence the relative enjoyment of the sexual passage. Specifically, subjects who had been primed by visualizing non-permissive significant others (i.e., parents) reported enjoying the sexually permissive passage less than their counterparts who had been primed by visualizing permissive significant others (peers). In a similar investigation (Baldwin, et al., 1990; Study I), graduate students evaluated their own research ideas after a 2 ms (subliminal) exposure to a slide depicting either a disapproving authority figure (department chair) or an approving non—authority figure (post-doctoral fellow). Results indicated that subjects evaluated their research ideas 59 in a more positive manner after exposure to the approving prime relative to the disapproving stimulus. Extending this research, the authors (Baldwin, et a1, 1990: Study 2) had Catholic women read a sexually permissive passage (Baldwin & Holmes, 1987) and then sublirninally presented them with a slide depicting either a disapproving significant other (Pope John Paul II) or a disapproving non-significant other (the department chair from another institution). After the exposure to the prime, each subject completed a self- evaluation measure. Results of this study replicated the previous finding that subliminal exposure to a disapproving image of a significant other (Pope John Paul II) resulted in lower self-evaluations. Furthermore, no priming effect was observed when the disapproving image of a non-significant other was used as the prime. Taken together, these studies demonstrate that positive and negative self-evaluations can be differentially primed by exposure to positive and negative prior social contexts. This effect, however, may be mediated by the degree to which the priming event is a relevant part of the subject's existing knowledge structures. Priming ef self-efficacy. The extent to which self-efficacy expectations can be directly altered by exposure to an unrelated context has not been investigated. If one assumes, however, that positive or negative self-schemata can be primed, it is possible to assess the indirect affect of priming on self-efficacy by examining the relationship between self-schemata and self-efficacy. Building on theoretical assumptions set forth by Kuiper, MacDonald and Derry (1983), Bruch and his colleagues (Bruch, Chesser & Meyer, 1989; Bruch, Meyer & Chesser, 1987) examined the role of evaluative self- 60 schemata in the prediction and evaluation of behavioral events. In their first study (Bruch, et al., 1987) the investigators demonstrated that individuals with negative self-schemas were more inclined to engage in pessimistic self-talk during performance compared to those with positive views of themselves. Subsequently, the authors (Bruch, et al., 1989) extended these findings by setting forth and testing the following line of reasoning: Self-schema might function as the locus where experiential data are stored at an abstracted and generalized level and must therefore be considered as a major source for self-efficacy. It can be assumed that their impact is further strengthened by the self- schema inherent bias mechanism which can be expected to suppress available situational information. (p. 74) In this study, subjects' evaluative self-schema was operationalized as the discrepancy between real (myself as I am) and ideal (myself as I would like to be) self. Specifically, subjects with a relatively high real self to ideal self ratio were classified as positive self-schematics; subjects with a relatively low real to ideal self ratio were classified as negative self-schematics. After introducing a moderate amount of stress into the situation by emphasizing the importance of a good performance on the upcoming tasks, subjects were asked to offer predictions as to how they would perform on the tasks. After making their efficacy judgments, subjects completed three cognitive performance tasks. The results, as predicted, revealed that negative self-schematics had significantly lower self-efficacy expectations relative to positive self-schematics. In addition, although not statistically significant, negative schematics exhibited somewhat lower 61 performance scores than did the positive schematics on the cognitive performance tasks. The authors discuss the implications of their findings as follows: In reference to Bandura's (1977) suggestions for processing of self—efficacy, our findings seem to suggest that achievement of performance accomplishments does not necessarily lead to enhancement of self—perceived mastery for coping achievement. In other words, if cognitive processing in self-regulation is at fault, potentially strong sources for enhancement of self—efficacy might not be sufficient to strengthen such expectations accordingly. Thus, to facilitate adaptive self-regulation via self- efficacy it appears to be crucial to challenge biased interference as supported by a negative self-schema. (p. 82) As discussed previously, Bandura's more recent writings (Bandura, 1986) have incorporated this position that none of four major sources of efficacy is inherently capable of generating efficacy expectations. Rather, it is the cognitive processing of the informatien from each of these sources that conveys an efficacy expectation. Therefore, efficacy expectations are the result of an individual selectively attending, weighting and integrating information relevant to the likelihood of successful performance on a particular task. Priming ef moeg states. It is worth noting that a review of the literature relative to priming and mood states produces more studies using alterations in mood as the priming (manipulating) mechanism than the primed (manipulated) variable. In fact, in any particular study, it can be difficult to discern whether mood states are the priming or the primed cognitive factor. For example, in the mood 62 induction studies cited earlier in this chapter, moods can either be thought of as being "primed" by having the individual recalling positive or negative events (Kavanagh & Bower, 1985; Wright 8: Mischel, 1982), giving them a small gift (Isen, et al., 1978), and by providing them with bogus feedback (Stanley 8: Maddux, 1986); or as priming subsequent dependent measures (e.g., self-efficacy on a upcoming task or the satisfaction with consumer products). Riskind (1989) further elaborates on this point that moods can serve as priming variables by stating the following: "When individual schemas or categories are primed by a mood-induction, such priming would enhance retrieval of schema—congruent material. . . priming by a mood-producing event may increase the encoder's readiness to elaborate schema -- or category -- congruent material. (p. 175) Some priming studies, however, have more explicitly defined mood as the dependent measure. Baldwin, et al. (1990; Study 2), for example, found that subliminal exposure to a photograph of a disapproving face was capable of producing a significantly negative affect. This result was only observed, however, when the face was that of a familiar and relevant authority figure. In addition, in their study examining the priming of affective self— perceptions, Skelton and Strohmetz (1990) found that subjects reported experiencing more physical symptoms of illness after they had been exposed to a health-related priming task. Specifically, subjects in the priming condition engaged in a task that required them to indicate which of two words "is more health-related -- makes you think more about health and illness." Subjects primed in this manner reported having more total symptoms of gastrointestinal, cold 63 and sinus, aches and pains, cardiovascular and pulmonary, and skin problems relative to subjects who had not taken part in the priming task. Although these studies seem to suggest mood states can be altered by priming, studies by Homstein and his colleagues have failed to demonstrate a priming effect on mood (e.g., Holloway, Tucker & Homstein, 1977; Homstein, LaKind, Frankel 8: Moore, 1975). In summary, relatively few studies have been specifically designed to investigate priming effects on self-referent cognitions. Those studies that do exist -- and those conducted in closely related area -- suggest that priming may be capable of having an affect on self-schema and self-efficacy. On the other hand, studies examining the effects of priming of mood states have generally produced equivocal results. Qegnitiens and Meter Perfermepee A number of cognitive factors such as anxiety (e.g., Bird & Horn, 1990; Pragman, 1986), attentional focus (e.g., Albrecht 8r Feltz, 1987; Nideffer, 1976; Van Schoyck & Grasha, 1981) and goal intentions (e.g., Hall 8r Byme, 1988: Locke, 1991; Smith & Lee, 1992) have been proposed to influence the quality of motor performance. Two additional cognitive variables that have received a great deal of attention in the sport psycholog literature are self-efficacy and mood states. :r-l. onhlo 9‘ “n ‘ -ffl e_. 00 0‘ aria! ' The intuitively appealing notion that task-specific confidence (self- efficacy) is positively correlated with performance has received overwhelming support from a number of empirical investigations. The robustness of the efficacy-performance relationship is evidenced by 64 the variety of settings in which it has been reported. Positive relationships between self-efficacy expectations and performance have been reported in such diverse motor activities as diving (Feltz, 1982; Feltz, 1988a; Feltz & Albrecht, 1986: Feltz, et al., 1979; Feltz & Mugno, 1983), leg endurance (Gould 81 Weiss, 1981; Weinberg et al., 1979; 1980; 1981), gmnastics (Lee, 1982; McAuley, 1985; McAuley & Gill, 1983; Weinberg, et al., 1982) reaction time and motor coordination (Ryckman, Robbins, Thornton 8r Cantrell, 1982), marathon running (Gayton, Matthews 8: Burchstead, 1986), tennis (Barling 8r Abel, 1983), handgrip strength (Kavanagh & Hausfeld, 1986), and golf putting (Woolfolk, Murphy, Gottesfeld 8r Aitken, 1985). Although several hypotheses can be set forth to explain the positive relationship between self-efficacy expectations and performance, Bandura (1986) has offered the following: "Those who judge themselves inefficacious in coping with environmental demands dwell upon their personal deficiencies and cognize potential difficulties as more formidable than they really are. . . . Research shows that people who regard themselves as highly efficacious act, think, and feel differently from those who perceive themselves as inefficacious. (pp. 394-395) W. The correlation between mood states and motor performance has been investigated so frequently in the past two decades that LeUnes, Hayward & Daiss (1989) were able to publish an annotated bibliography limited to only those studies examining the use of a single mood assessment instrument -- the Profile of Mood States (POMS; McNair, et al., 1971) -- in sport-related contexts. They reported that 65 between 1975 and 1988, the POMS was cited in 56 sport-related papers, covering 19 different sport areas. During that l3-year period, articles were published examining POMS scores of athletes participating in such diverse sports as distance running (Tharion, Strowman 8r Rauch, 1988), track and field (Thaxton, 1982) exercise and fitness (Bahrke, Thompson (Sr Thomas, 1986) football (Daus, Wilson 8: Freeman, 1986), swimming and diving (Berger & Owen, 1988; Morgan, Brown, Raglin, O'Conner 81 Ellickson, 1987), wrestling (Nagle, Morgan, Hellickson, Serfass 8: Alexander, 1975), body building (Freedson, Mihevic, Loucks & Girandola, 1983; Fuchs & Zaichkowsky, 1983), cycling (Hagberg, Mullin, Bahrke & Limburg, 1979; Johnson, et al., 1985), wheelchair athletics (Henschen, Horvat 81 French, 1984), basketball (Craighead, Privette, Vallianos & Byrkit, 1986), gmnastics (Edwards 8: Huston, 1984), karate (McGowan & Jordon, 1988), netball (Miller 8: Miller, 1985), rowing (Morgan & Johnson, 1978), rodeo participation (Meyers, Sterling & LeUnes, 1988), soccer (Robinson & Howe, 1987) speed skating (Gutman, Pollock, Foster 81 Schmidt, 1984), and volleyball (Wilson, Ainsworth & Bird, 1985). The popular and scientific writings of Morgan and his colleagues (e.g., Farrell, Gates, Maksud & Morgan, 1982; Morgan, 1980a; 1980b; Morgan, et al., 1987; Morgan & Horstman, 1978; Morgan 8: Johnson, 1978; Morgan, O'Connor, Ellickson 8r Bradley, 1988; Morgan & Pollock, 1977; Nagle, et al., 1975) have done much to enhance the understanding of mood affects and motor performance. Perhaps Morgan's most important contribution has been to identify, describe and popularize the characteristics of a POMS profile frequently shown to be associated with superior motor performance -- the so-called 66 "iceberg profile" (Morgan, 1980a; 1980b). As Morgan (1980b) describes it, the iceberg profile exists when "athletes score below the 50th T-Score on tension, depression, fatigue, and confusion, and above the 50th T-Score on vigor in comparison with published norms" (p. 63). A visual presentation of the iceberg profile is depicted in Figure 1 which plots actual POMS data collected from elite runners, wrestlers and oarsmen (Morgan & Pollock, 1977). Similar profiles have been shown by other investigators to be associated with successful body builders (fieedson, et al., 1983; Fuchs 8r Zaichkowsky, 1983), cyclists, (Hagberg, et al., 1979; Johnson, et al., 1985), football players (Nation & LeUnes, 1981), swimmers (Riddick, 1984), weightlifters (Mahoney, 1989) and wheelchair athletes (Henschen, et al., 1984). Conversely, the absence of the iceberg profile has recently been associated with athletic "staleness" (Morgan, et al., 1987). Although not universally supported by the empirical evidence (e.g., Craighead, et al., 1986; Daiss, LeUnes & Nation, 1986; Miller & Miller, 1985), the iceberg profile has gained considerable acceptance in the sport community as a correlate to elite performance. One example of the extremes to which the iceberg profile is sometimes considered the "gold-standard" mood profile for effective athletic performance is demonstrated by the women's cross-country track team at Brigham Young University (BYU). Not only did the team implement the POMS as part of a diagnostic testing program, but it was expected that athletes with POMS profiles differing from the iceberg profile undergo some form of intervention such as relaxation, or cognitive behavioral rehearsal (Poole & Henschen, 1984). 67 TEN DEP AN G VIG FAT CON POMS FACTOR Figpre 1. Classic "iceberg" profile exhibited by elite runners, oarsmen and wrestlers. Adapted from Morgan 8r Pollock (197 7). 68 Although it is frequently assumed (as the case of the BYU cross- country team attests) that it is an athlete's mood that has an effect on his or her performance, most of the studies mentioned thus far are only capable of demonstrating a correlational, rather than a causal, relationship between mood and performance. Several studies, in fact, have found that the reverse is true. That is, manipulations in performance cause changes in mood states (e.g., Berger 8r Owen, 1988; Kowal, Patton & Vogel, 1978; Lichtrnan 8: Poser, 1983; Wilfley 8r Kunce, 1986, etc.). Even if evidence could be assembled supporting the intuitively appealing notion that mood exerts a tremendous influence on the quality of motor performance, the cognitive mechanisms involved are. as yet, undetermined. It remains to be demonstrated, for example, whether mood exerts its influence on performance via changes in self- schemata or self-efficacy separately, via both schemata and efficacy or through some other mechanism entirely. The relatienship bemeen priming £le behavier. Although the effects of priming, per se, have yet to be investigated in regard to neuro-muscular performance, Anshel's (1988) investigation into the relationships among self-schemata, mood states, feedback and motor skill performance offers preliminary support for the contention that motor performance may be susceptible to cognitive priming. Despite being of relatively minor importance to the purposes of his study. Anshel reported that subjects within whom a negative mood had been primed (Anshel never uses this term) by reading negative self-referent statements (Velten, 1968) exhibited poorer performance on a pursuit 69 rotor task than subjects who had been primed with either positive or neutral self-referent statements. Two additional studies that involve priming of behavior in sport and recreational contexts have been conducted by Worchel (1972) and Josephson (1981; reported in Berkowitz 8r Rogers. 1986). In the Josephson study, young boys were intentionally frustrated and then made to watch scenes from a popular television program. Half of the boys saw a violent scene in which the villains used walkie-talkies during the commission of their violence. The remaining subjects watched a non-violent scene from the same program. After priming, all boys played a game of floor hockey which allowed them to act out their aggressive tendencies. During some games, the referees carried walkie-talkies which presumably would serve as retrieval cues for subjects who had been exposed to the violent scene. Results indicated that boys who had viewed the violent television program and then played in a floor hockey games in which the referees carried walkie- talkies acted in the most aggressive manner. Worchel (1972) primed young summer campers by having them view either a film of a prize fight or a film of a boating trip. After administering the prime, subjects were given the opportunity to indicate whether they would rather participate in a pie fight or take a ride on a raft. Campers who had watched the prize fight were more likely to choose to participate in the pie fight while those who had seen the film of the boating trip indicated they would prefer to go on the raft ride. Additional support can be found for the priming of other overt physical behaviors. Carver, Ganellen, Froming and Chambers (1983), 70 in fact, provide evidence that some behaviors previously attributed to vicarious learning associated with modeling may actually be the result of schematic priming. In the first of two experiments examining this priming explanation for modeling effects, the investigators exposed subjects to either a hostile or neutral videotaped interaction between a businessman and his secretary. In accordance with protocol set forth by Srull and Wyer (1979), subjects later read a paragraph depicting an individual engaging in a number of ambiguous behaviors such as refusing to pay his rent until his landlord fixed the plumbing. Finally, subjects rated the degree to which they thought the person in the paragraph exhibited hostile behaviors. Results indicated that subjects exposed to the videotape of the hostile social interaction rated the target person as being more hostile. The findings of this first experiment indicated that it is possible to use the behavior of a medfl (as opposed to one's own actions) to prime interpretive knowledge structures. In their second study (Carver, et al., 1983; Study 2) the investigators were no longer interested in priming hostile or aggressive atmpdes -- something they (and others, e.g., Srull 81 Wyer, 1979) had already demonstrated -- but rather in the priming of aggressive behavier. Borrowing heavily on the research protocol made famous by Stanley Milgram (1963) in his classic work on obedience to authority, subjects were paired with a confederate under the guise of a "learning experiment." The subject was always assigned the role of "teacher" and the confederate was always the "learner." Subjects were to teach various concepts to their partner (the confederate) by rewarding (correct) and punishing (incorrect) his responses. Prior to 71 the teaching task, however, the subject was asked to take part in another experiment (while they waited for the conveniently late- arriving confederate). During this "unrelated" scrambled sentence task, subjects engaged in Srull and Wyer's (1979) sentence formation task which involved creating meaningful sentences from sets of words. Each of the 30 four-word sets was constructed so that only two possible complete sentences could be formed. All sentences that could be formed were of either hostile (e.g., "leg break arm his") or neutral content (e. g., "her found know 1"). Subjects randomly assigned to the hostile priming condition completed 24 (80%) hostile sentences and 6 (20%) neutral sentences. These percentages were reversed for subjects in the neutral priming condition. After the introduction of the prime, subjects returned to their primary task of teaching through the use of rewards and punishments. During the bogus learning task, the subject asked his partner 34 questions. If the response was correct (14 times), the subject rewarded the learner by turning on a "correct" light. If the answer was incorrect (20 times), the subject used his discretion to administer one of 10 linearly increasing shock intensities. Results indicated that subjects exposed to the hostile primes gave significantly more intense shocks to their incorrect partners than did control (neutral prime) subjects. There is an apparent connection between the work of Carver and his colleagues (Carver, et al., 1983) and the broader literature involving "aggressive cueing" (e.g., Berkowitz & Alioto, 1973; Berkowitz & Green, 1967). In fact, when Carver, et al. discuss the notion of aggressive cueing in light of their research, they specifically mention the Berkowitz and Green study where provoked subjects 72 watch a fight scene from a movie. Ravin and Rubin (1976) succinctly summarize this research as follows: Subjects were shown a particularly bloody seven-minute scene from the film Chempien, in which a boxer, played by the actor Kirk Douglas, was beaten to a pulp. Some subjects were then introduced to a confederate named "Kir "; other subjects were introduced to the same confederate, but in their case he was called "Bob." When named ","Kirk the confederate was much more apt to be given severe shocks by the subjects. (p. 138) Although Berkowitz (1974) originally hypothesized these and similar findings to be the result of classical conditioning, he has recently come to favor a "cognitive-neoassociationistic" (e.g., Anderson 8r Bower, 1973) or priming explanation. After reviewing a number of fascinating studies in which highly publicized acts of violence seem to serve as triggers for similar violence, Berkowitz and Rogers (1986) conclude: All this research suggests that media depictions of violence can prompt people in the audience to act aggressively toward others or themselves, even when the portrayals of violence are fictional. . . . We propose that when people witness, read, or hear of an event via the mass media, ideas fie eetivgted whieh, fer a sheg; ri of e ten evoke other m r l h (p. 57-58; italics added) A series of controlled experiments conducted by Homstein and his colleagues (e.g., Blackman 8r Homstein, 1977; Holloway, et al., 1977; Homstein, et al., 1975) appears to lend support to Berkowitz's contention that priming can activate cognitions which subsequently 73 find expression as aggressive behaviors toward others. In these studies, subjects' behavior toward others was observed after being exposed to positive or negative news broadcasts while they waited to take part in a decision-making study. Homsten, et al. (1975; Study 1), for example, activated subjects' pro-social or anti-social cognitions by priming them with news stories that depicted either the "best" (a man donated a kidney to save the life of stranger) or "worst" (a man strangled a 72 year old woman) aspects of human nature. Following the priming, subjects participated in a nonzero sum competition/cooperation (prisoner's dilemma) game. As predicted, subjects primed with the positive news story behaved in a more cooperative manner -- and expected more cooperation from their partner -- than did subjects primed with the negative news story. A similar investigation conducted by Holloway, et al. (1977) indirectly exposed subjects to radio broadcasts containing good (human lives were saved) and bad (human lives were lost) news that was either social (the result of intentional human action) or nonsocial (the result of an act of nature) in content. After the priming manipulation, subjects engaged in a prisoner's dilemma game in which they chose to either cooperate or compete with an unseen partner for additional money. Results indicated a significant interaction with cooperative behavior being elicited most often by exposure to the positive social news and competitive behavior being elicited most often by exposure to the negative social news. In summary, although no studies have been designed to specifically assess the effect of positive and negative priming on motor behavior, there is considerable evidence to suggest that behavior -- in 74 general -- is susceptible to the effects of cognitive priming. The present investigation was undertaken, therefore, to extend these findings to the area of motor behavior. CHAPTER III METHOD Pre-Experimental Schematicig Assessment According to Markus (197 7), self-schematicity for a particular attribute is a combined function of the extent to which the individual feels the attribute is self-descriptive, and the degree to which the attribute is perceived as being important to the individual's self-image. Using these two dimensions of self-description and importance as criteria by which to categorize individuals' schematicity for exercising, Kendzierski (1988) operationally defined individuals who perceived an attribute (exercising) as being both extremely self-descriptive and important as having a positive schema. Similarly, individuals perceiving this attribute as being extremely non-descriptive but important were categorized as having a negative self-schemata. A final category of "aschematic" was used to describe subjects who thought the attribute to be only moderately self-descriptive or non-descriptive and low in importance. Employing a methodolog consistent with that used by Kendzierski, potential subjects were assessed regarding their self-schematicity for physical ability. Subjects As part of the procedure for recruiting and screening potential subjects for participation in the experimental manipulation phase of the investigation, 1018 female undergraduates attending any of five introductory psycholog classes at Michigan State University were surveyed as to their schematicity for physical ability. In the first two classes, data were collected from all students in attendance. However, because only females were to be included in the experimental phase of 7 5 76 the study, it was determined that continuing to collect screening data from male subjects would be inefficient and costly. Only female subjects were screened in the final three classes and only data from females were used in the classification of subjects’ self-schemata. In terms of academic class standing, 51.6% of the female respondents classified themselves as freshmen, 26.5% sophomores, 14.9% juniors and 7.0% seniors. In n n Perceived Physical Abiligg Scale lPPAmes))° Respondents' self- descriptions of their physical abilities were obtained by administering the 10-item Perceived Physical Ability (PPA) subscale of the Physical Self-Efficacy Scale (Ryckman, et al., 1982). Although items contained in the original PPA (e.g., "I have excellent reflexes"; "I am not agile and graceful"; "I take pride in my ability in sports") are measured on a 6- point Likert-type scale (1 = strongly agree; 6 = strongly disagree) possible responses in the present study were modified by expanding the scale to 7-points (allowing for a midpoint response) and changing the anchors to read: (1) this describes me; (7) this does NOT describe me. (See Appendix A for a complete copy of the self-descriptive version of the PPA.) Im ortance of P r iv Ph si Abili S a1 PP imp))' The importance subjects placed on having each of the physical abilities described in the PPA was assessed by administering a second modified version of the lO-item PPA. Items' stems and alternatives were modified so as to reflect an importance dimension. For example, the original self-description items "I have excellent reflexes" and "I am not agile and gracef " were modified to read "Having excellent reflexes" 77 and "Being agile and graceful" and were measured on a 7 -point Likert- type scale with anchors of: (1) this is very important to me; (7) this is m very important to me. (See Appendix B for a complete copy of the importance version of the PPA.) Marlew-Qrewne Seeiel Desirabilijy Seals. Given the self-report nature of the physical ability scales, subjects' self-descriptions and importance ratings of their physical abilities may be, in part, the result of intentional or unintentional response bias in favor of socially desirable behavior. Therefore, each subject's tendency to respond in a socially desirable manner was assessed by administering a l3-item (Form C) shortened version (Reynolds, 1982) of the Marlow-Crowne Social Desirability Scale (Crowne 8r Marlow, 1960). (See Appendix C for a complete copy of the shortened version of the Marlow-Crowne Social Desirability Scale.) Wm 11 n f h i 111 hm i . Onthefirstdayof class, students attending introductory psycholog courses were given information by their professor and a psycholog department representative regarding an option to participate in several departmentally approved psycholog research projects in return for partial course credit. After the requirements for this option were fully explained, the students were told the experimenter was currently involved in such a research project and needed to collect pre- experimental "screening information" from the class. Individuals meeting the criteria for participation in the actual study would then be contacted by telephone to arrange a mutually convenient time to 78 conduct the experimental phase of the investigation. Because a complete understanding of the true purpose of the study would have seriously jeopardized the validity of the results, subjects were told only that the information they were providing was part of an ongoing investigation into the perceptions college students have regarding their physical ability. A questionnaire packet including: (a) the Perceived Physical Ability Scale, (b) the Importance of Physical Ability Scale and (c) a shortened version (Reynolds, 1982; Form C) of the Marlow-Crowne Social Desirability Scale (Crowne & Marlow, 1960) was distributed to each student. When they had completed the three instruments. the students deposited their questionnaire packet in a collection box. 1. ifi .. n f u ' ' lf- hm for h i .. .pilia. Female students demonstrating a tendency toward socially desirable responses were excluded from further analyses. For the purposes of this investigation, a tendency toward making socially desirable responses was operationally defined as a total score above the sample mean of 5.43 (s.d. = 2.83) on the shortened version of the Marlow- Crowne Social Desirability Scale (Reynolds, 1982). On the basis of this criterion, 505 (49.6%) of the 1018 female subjects were defined as responding in a socially desirable manner and were therefore eliminated from the study. Sample means and standard deviations for perceived physical ability and the importance placed on these abilities were then calculated for the remaining 513 female subjects. Based on their responses to the self-description and importance of physical ability scales, subjects were placed into one of the four mutually exclusive physical ability self-schema categories presented in Table 3. 79 .05": u .womuco .mmnan .mmfiucm 26am beam: wouomfimo 3886 05 mo than 85 wfizfi «o: 8.80m nofimmfloéoz 6 mafia 55 32 use 3 .om ~85 $2 wwfim :9: 83.3% o 832552 9 .om can... 539% mode .85 $2 a monmfionom gummuz 9 .0m $5 559% mode 55 ~onon w moagonom 2568 38m 853.593 0.80m coamuowonfiow bowaO «Eonom ban? 3223 hi? Rosana 589.80an ban... new?“ .8 8589390 «a 5 cosoaaa 83m season .m «Ems 80 These categories are in keeping with previous literature which has consistently emphasize the need to consider both self-description and importance dimensions in the classification of an individual's self- schema (Kendzierski, 1988; 1990; Markus, 1977). Kendzierski (1988) describes the way these two dimensions are combined to categorize self-schema as follows: As defined by Markus (1977) individuals are schematic in regard to a particular attribute when they consider that attribute to be either extremely self-descriptive (i.e., positive physical ability schematics] . . . or extremely non-descriptive (i.e., negative physical ability schematics] . . . and when they consider that attribute to be extremely important to their self-image. Likewise, individuals are considered aschematic when they consider the attribute to be only moderately descriptive or nondescriptive and they do not consider the attribute to be important to their self- image. (pp. 46-47) ical ili h m ti were operationally defined as those subjects with both PPAmes) and PPA(11np) scores above the group mean (PPA(deS) x = 43.03, g = 10.19; PPA(1mp) x = 50.16, s_d = 11.15). Negapve physieal ability schematies were defined as subjects with PPAmes) scores below, and PPA(imp) scores above the group mean. Subjects were defined as being aschernaficimariphxsiealam when their PPA(des) scores ranged between 1.0 standard deviation above to 1.0 standard deviation below the group mean and their PPAump) scores fell below the group mean. Finally, any subject with physical ability self-description and importance scores placing them outside the three previously defined schema categories were 8 1 considered as having schemata aetflassified for the purposes of the present study. E rim n D 11 §iibJth§ Based on their physical ability schematicity classification, 81 female volunteers between the ages of 18 and 26 were recruited to serve as subjects in the experimental phase of the investigation. Because a completely randomized block design was employed, 27 subjects were selected from each of the following schematicity categories: (a) positive self-schema for physical activity, (b) negative self-schema for physical activity and (c) aschematic toward physical activity. Subjects were given course credit in exchange for participation. Because a complete understanding of the true purpose of the investigation would have seriously jeopardized the validity of the results, subjects were recruited ostensibly to take part in a nationwide study to develop performance norms for college students on a variety of motor tasks. In n n W. Information pertaining to each subject's demographic and sports-related background was collected by administering an 1 1-item demographic / background questionnaire. Demographic information gathered included: subject's name, age, racial / ethnic affiliation, academic class, approximate grade point average and college major. Additional questions designed to assess the subject's previous sport experience were included in the 82 demographic / background questionnaire. (See Appendix D for a complete copy of the demographic /background questionnaire.) W. To assess the impact that exposure to a positive, negative or neutral social environment has on the subjects' mood state, each subject completed a 37-item abbreviated version (Shacham, 1983) of the Profile of Mood States (McNair, et al., 1971). The original POMS consists of a set of 65 adjectives (e.g., "tense", "miserable", "muddled", "listless", etc.) or phrases (e.g., "sorry for things done", "ready to fight", "uncertain about things", etc.) developed to assess the six "transient, fluctuating affective states" of tension, depression, anger, fatigue, confusion, and vigor (McNair, et al., 1971, p. 5). The extent to which subjects felt a particular adjective or phrase described how they were feeling at the time was indicated on a 5-point Likert-type scale (0 = not at all, 1 = a little, 2 = moderately, 3 = quite a bit, 4 = extremely). Subjects expressing difficulty understanding the meaning of any particular POMS item were given a synonymous item from the POMS alternative word lists developed by Albrecht 8r Ewing (1989). (See Appendix E for a complete copy of the abbreviated version of the Profile of Mood States and Appendix F for a copy of the alternative word lists.) Physiea; ability self-sehemata. An indication of the respondents' self-schema for physical ability (subj ects' self-descriptions and the importance they placed on these physical abilities) was obtained by administering the same lO-item self descriptions and 10-item importance ratings derived from the Perceived Physical Ability (PPA) subscale of the Physical Self-Efficacy Scale (Ryckman, et al., 1982) used in the pre-experimental schematicity assessment phase of the 83 study. Subj ects' self-schematicity for physical ability was computed by multiplying each self description item by the importance rating for that item and summing the 10 products. Task-speeifie self-efficacy. Because previous "research from the sport literature provides clear evidence that a significant relationship exists between self-confidence [self-efficacy] and performance" (Feltz, 1988b; p. 451), a task-specific (golf putting task) measure of self- efficacy was constructed that enabled a microanalytic research methodolog (Bandura, 1977; 1986) to be employed when "testing propositions about the origins and functions of perceived self-efficacy" (Bandura, 1986, p. 422). "Specifically, each subject was asked to indicate, the strength of belief that she would be successful in hitting at least 1 out of 20 golf putts in such a manner as to make the ball come to rest within 12 in. (30.48 cm) of a specified target; at least 2 out of 20 golf putts within 12 in. (30.48 cm) of the target; at least 3 out of 20 golf putts within 12 in. (30.48 cm) of the target; all the way up to the maximum -- hitting all 20 out of 20 golf putts within 12 in. (30.48 cm) of the target. Each efficacy rating was made on a 10-point probability scale ranging from 0 (I am certain I eappet do this) to 10 (I am certain I 9&1. do this). (See Appendix G for a copy of the task- specific measure of self-efficacy.) Qelf putting task. Psychomotor performance was assessed via a golf putting task. The putting task consisted of 20 trials in which the subject putted a regulation golf ball a distance of 3.05 m, along a carpeted floor, toward a 2.54 cm by 2.54 cm square target that was formed by the intersection of two pieces of masking tape (each 1.83 m long and 2.54 cm wide) crossing at their midpoint (Figure 2). Both 84 Videotape Recorder (on tripod) 10 feet (3.05 m) ' ’ mere“: . . go eac W. Golf putting performance task. 85 pieces of tape were marked at 1 in. (2.54 cm) intervals. Each subject was tested individually and no time limits were imposed. All trials were recorded on videotape thereby eliminating the need for an experimenter to be present during the performance. Error associated with each putt was computed by summing the shot's vertical and horizontal deviation from the target's midpoint. Although several types of error scores were considered (e.g., variable error (VE), constant error (CE), total error (E), etc.; absolute error (AE) which, according to Spray (1986) "seems to be the strongest indicator of target accuracy under most conditions" (p. 237) was assumed to be the single best indicator of overall golf putting accuracy. This assumption was also based on the fact that absolute error "is a composite score that is mathematically derived from both CE and VE" (Magill, 1985, p. 31). The first five shots of the pre-test and the last five shots of the post-test were eliminated from the total error score in order to control for habituation and fatigue effects, respectively. Qeaehing style videegpes. Three 5-minute VHS cassette videotapes were produced, each depicting a female coach addressing her basketball team. The same actor played the role of the coach in all three tapes. In two of the tapes, the coach commented on, and made references to, her team's performance. The only difference between the two tapes was that in one, the coach was shown employing a "negative" coaching style (e. g., criticizing and ridiculing the players for their poor performance) while in the other, the coach approached the same situation from a "positive" coaching perspective (encouraging, and reinforcing the players for the things they had done correctly). The third tape depicted the coach providing her team with 86 "emotionally neutr " organizational information (such as the time the team bus will be leaving the school, etc.). (See Appendix H for a transcription of the content of the three manipulation videotapes.) Upon completion, each videotape was reviewed using a modified version of the Coaching Behavior Assessment System (CBAS; Smith, Smoll & Hunt, 1977) to insure that: (a) the positive coaching style videotape contained the highest proportion of "mistake-contingent encouragements" relative to other coded behaviors, (b) the negative coaching style videotape contained the highest proportion of "punishments" and "punitive" behaviors relative to other coded behaviors, and (c) the neutral coaching videotape contained the highest proportion of "organizational" behaviors relative to other coded behaviors. Although the original CBAS consists of 12 behavioral categories, only those related to "reactions to mistakes" and "organization" were coded. (See Appendix I for a description of the CBAS categories.) Mapipulapen Check Questionnaire. A 12-item questionnaire was administered to each subject at the end of the data collection session. Specifically, the items were designed to assess (a) the degree to which the subject was aware of the true purpose of the study, (b) the degree to which the subject was aware of the manipulation, and (c) the subject's perception of the effect the manipulation had on her performance. In addition to these three questions of interest, several irrelevant "filler" items were included in the manipulation check I questionnaire in an attempt to cenceal the true nature of the questionnaire. (See Appendix J for a copy of the Manipulation Check Questionnaire.) 87 Preeedure Qelleetien ef eaperimental data. At a pre-arranged time, each subject reported directly to the testing room. When the subject entered the room, the experimenter was seated in front of a 21-inch T.V. monitor, viewing a videotape of a physical education instructor giving a lecture. The instructor was presenting emotionally "neutral" information pertaining to the historical development of the sub- specializations of exercise physiolog and biomechanics within the field of physical education (See Appendix H for a transcription of the lecture content). The experimenter was using a checklist to record information from the videotaped lecture. After introducing himself, the experimenter gave each subject a brief overview of the bogus study (development of college student motor performance norms) and informed them as to what their am participation in the current study would entail. Each subject was taken into an adjacent room and asked to stand behind the shooting line in order to get "a feel" for the task demands. Subjects were not misled regarding any of the tasks in which they would be participating (only the specific purpose of the study was concealed). After completing an informed consent form (Appendix K) and the demographic and background questionnaire, each subject was asked to sit quietly for five minutes. Ostensibly, the purpose of this rest period was to "standardize the conditions under which the golf putting tests were administered." Each subject was given a BIODOT biofeedback button to place on the back of her hand and a small reference card explaining the degree of stress associated with each color. (See Appendix L for a copy of the BIODOT color reference chart.) The sole 88 purpose of the BIODOT button was to reinforce the legitimacy of the rest period. While the subject was "resting," the experimenter returned to his previous task of coding the videotaped instruction. The audio on the T.V. monitor was set so the subject could clearly overhear the instructor's voice. The subject was seated at a 45-degree angle to the T.V., approximately 1.83 m from the screen. This positioning was selected for its "natural" appearance -- not forcing the subjects to look directly into the monitor, yet putting the monitor in plain view should they choose to look at the screen during the resting period. After 5 minutes of "resting," each subject completed (a) the abbreviated version of the POMS, (b) the PPA(des) and PPAflmp) scales and (c) the task-specific self-efficacy scale. To control for possible order effects, presentation order of the three instruments was randomly counterbalanced between each of the six possible sequences (i.e., A—B-C; A-C-B; B-A-C; B-C-A: C-A-B; and C-B-A). When the pre- manipulation questionnaires were completed, the experimenter escorted the subject into the adjacent room and asked her to putt, at her own pace, 20 golf balls toward the target, trying to get the balls to come to a stop as close as possible to the center of the target. In order to reduce the likelihood of the balls hitting one another, subjects were asked to "clear the target area" after putting each cup of balls (5 balls) toward the target. Subjects were responsible for clearing the target because the experimenter was not present during the golf putting performance. This was done to remove the possibility of the experimenter providing subtle positive or negative feedback or reinforcement cues to the subjects during their performance. Instead. 89 each performance was recorded using a video camera placed at a 45- degree angle from the target and aimed away from the subject (See Figure 2). Using the video camera to record performance, not only eliminated the possibility of experimenter cues, and allowed a more careful analysis of each performance, it also reduced the likelihood of social facilitation performance (audience) effects resulting from self- presentation (Bond, 1982) evaluation apprehension (Cottrell, 1972), distraction (Baron, 1986) or the mere presence of the investigator (Zajonc, 1965). After starting the camera, the experimenter returned to the T.V. room under the guise of continuing his coding duties. Upon completion of the 20 putts, subjects returned to the T.V. room. Experimental manipulation and data eellectien. While the subject was performing the pre-manipulation golf putting task, the experimenter determined the manipulation group (positive, negative or neutral) to which the subject would be assigned. This was done by rolling a single die. If either 1 or 4 "pips" of the die were exposed, the subject was assigned to the positive coaching condition. If either 2 or 5 pips appeared, the subject was assigned to the negative coaching condition and if 3 or 6 pips were rolled, the subject was assigned to the neutral coaching condition. The only restriction was that each cell in the 3 X 3 randomized block matrix (Figure 3) contain an equal number of subjects. When the subjects returned to the T.V. room, the experimenter was again seated in front of the T.V. monitor. He was now, however, viewing a videotape of a basketball coach addressing her team. The videotape being viewed was determined by the group to which the subject had been randomly assigned. Consistent with the pre-manipulation procedures, the experimenter 90 .36353858 53% 3393.— "ofiaflab mace—e 2:53:90.— 53 E306 Hoe—n confine—=8.— h—ouoaaoo .wlolnnsuflm Suz an: Ru: sun: .3 . n . . i . . HO.“ E E E E E E 5332—5802... ha E E E E E E E a.» E amaze s... E E E E E E E . awwfim E E E E E E E ..25soz.. EEE EEE EEE aware a... E E E E E E E afifi E .. E E E E E . E .2338. :33 fines—8a 3 83585 .. 5:3 :33 $38.23 .58 anus—man had—532:0: ”53m wings 5:325» magma—n bane—now :33 gene 33> fleece gone 33> £230 25% 82> % N: : gal—.30. 91 was again using a checklist to record information related to the coaches' behavior. In accord with the pre-manipulation rest period, each subject was again asked to sit quietly for 5 minutes to "standardize the administration of the second set of self-perception and motor tasks." A new BIODOT biofeedback button was placed on the back of each subject's hand. In keeping with the pre-test protocol, the experimenter returned to his previous task of "coding" the videotape while the subject "rested." Again, the audio on the T.V. monitor was set so the subject could clearly overhear the coach's voice. During the 5-minute resting period, the subject was unwittingly exposed to the videotaped orations of either the negative, positive or neutral coach (depending on the experimental condition to which they had been assigned). After 5 minutes of exposure to either the positive, negative or neutral coach, the subject again completed (a) the abbreviated version of the POMS, (b) the PPA(des) and PPA(imp) scales and (c) the task-specific self-efficacy scale. The three instruments were administered in the same order in which they were presented during the pre-test. When the instruments were completed, the subjects were again escorted into the adjacent room and asked to putt 20 golf balls toward the target. The testing of each subject's performance was identical to that set forth during the pre-test. After completing the golf putting task, the subject once again returned to the T.V. room to complete the Manipulation Check Questionnaire and for debriefing. Mer_1ipuletien Qheck Questionnaire ad debriefing. Upon returning to the T.V. room, the subject completed the Manipulation 92 Check Questionnaire. Before being dismissed, each subject was informed as to the true purpose of the study. In addition, subjects were apprised of the fact that: Exposure to the videotape may have resulted in a change in the way you view yourself or performed on the golf task. You should not consider the perceptions you had of yourself or your performance on the motor task to be true indications of your abilities. Because others were still participating, subjects were asked not to disclose the purpose of the study to anyone for at least two weeks. W. All data collected in the study were coded, verified and keypunched into a "flat" data file on the IBM 3090 VF mainframe computer at Michigan State University. Statistical analyses were generated using SPSS" version 3.0 (SPSS Inc.) implemented through the IBM 3090 mainframe computer at Michigan State University. Because this study had as its purpose examining the differential effects positive and negative coaching has on a non—targeted observer's self-schemata, mood state, self-efficacy and motor performance, several analyses were required. In addition to the calculation of general descriptive statistics (for the entire sample and each of the experimental conditions), reliability coefficients were computed for each of the self-schema measures and the POMS subscales. A series of inferential statistics were also used to examine the overall integrity of the research design. Specifically, multivariate analysis of variance (MAN OVA) and oneway analysis of variance (ANOVA), with appropriate post hoc tests, were employed to assess mean differences between the 93 three experimental conditions prior to the experimental manipulation. Hypotheses regarding the effect of priming on self-schema, self- efficacy, mood state and performance were tested using a MANOVA formulated to test a doubly multivariate with repeated measures design (i.e., multiple dependent measures collected at multiple times). As a method of controlling for individual variation, changes between pre- and post-manipulation scores on the variables of interest were used as dependent measures. In addition, path analytic techniques (correlation and regression) were used to test the hypothesized causal relationships among the variables. All analyses employed an alpha level of .05. CHAPTER IV RESULTS AND DISCUSSION Due to the nature of the study design and the fact that several research hypotheses were tested, pre- and post-manipulation results will be presented separately. In general, pre-manipulation analyses were conducted to assess the overall integrity of the research design. Post-manipulation analyses, on the other hand, were primarily conducted to test the research hypotheses. Pr -M 111 n R Eggivalenge Qf Experimentg group fisignment Descriptive statistics for the four pre-manipulation outcome variables (i.e., self-schema, mood state, self-efficacy and performance) are presented in Table 4. A one-way MANOVA revealed that prior to the experimental manipulation, no statistically significant differences existed among the three experimental groups on any of the outcome variables (Wilks' lambda E (8,150) = 0.59, p = .78). Because the magnitude of the intercorrelations among the dependent variables may, in some cases, result in "the seemingly anomalous situation in which the MANOVA is nonsignificant but some or even all of the ANOVPs are significant" (Bray & Maxwell, 1985; p. 35), the univariate "protected" E-tests were also examined. None of the univariate tests were found to be statistically significant (self-schema E (2,78) = 0.54, p = 0.58; mood state E (2,78) = 1.98, n = .14; self-efficacy E (2,78) = 0.08, n = .92; performance E (2,78) = 0.13, n = .87). It was thus demonstrated that in regard to the variables of interest, the random assignment of subjects to the three experimental conditions resulted in generally equivalent groups. 94 95 A889 8.88 $98. 848 .88. $.83 E80: 8.8:. 858858 68.8. 8.2: 808. 05.8 888. 8.82 :88. 8.82 586m 68.0: 8.8 $5: 8.8 88.2. 8.8 38. 2.8 882 38. 2989 2.88 88.8. 2.88 8.3 8.88 88.88. 8.88 «838 8.8. 28¢ $8.88 $83 698. 8889. 86mm. 889 maggotom 5.3 8:: $8.88 3.88 88.8. 8.82 8.8.3 2.9: 586m and: 8.8 83: 3.8 5.9 $8 38. 8.8 882 888052 88.3 2.88 68.8. $.me $8. 8888 889 842 «805m 5.88. 88¢ .38.. whmfi. 68.8. 88¢ 88.2 5 88¢ 855.88 5.8. 8.88 89$. 882 .888. 8.8 88.8. :82 888m 88838 Eva: 8.8 83:. whom 38.8: 3.8 .88: 8.8 coo: 0.582 E8. 8.88 88.8. 8.88 858. 8.88 “38$ 8.8 «.80an $28. 8883 88.25 328 atom. 8.88 68.2: 88? 00551808 698. 8.2: 8.03 85.2 368. 3.82 888$ 8.x: 888m 838838 38. 3.8 68: 85.8 $5: 8.8 3.9 :8 822 9568 .8089 8.88 68.89 8.88 58.2.. 8.18 88.89 8.88 «80an a m a... m a m a... m ~58. £280 £800 #280 85:02 02832 258cm .moEMflSr Houseman con—83982-95 :< .8.“ monmnfim gantomofl .iv 2an 96 W A second MANOVA was conducted to determine if the three physical ability schematicity blocks (i.e., positive schematics, negative schematics, aschematics) differed on any of the outcome measures prior to the experimental manipulation. A significant omnibus MANOVA (Wilks' lambda E (8,150) = 8.03, p < .001) indicated that at least one statistically significant difference existed among the three schematicity blocks. A follow-up examination of the univariate E-tests revealed that the only significant pre-manipulation difference among the three blocks was on the self-schema variable (E (2,78) = 33.22, p_ < .001). Because the three schematicity blocks were constructed solely on the basis of differences on the self—schema measure (albeit several weeks earlier), this result was anticipated. An a posteriori multiple comparison test (Tukey’s studentized range test) revealed that positive schematics (1‘: = 336.78, 551 = 65.03) differed significantly from the other two schematicity groups (negative schematics x = 218.00, sd = 57 .77; aschematics i = 204.70, s1 = 72.83). Assessment ef Qrger Effeets The order in which the instruments measuring self-schema, mood state and self-efficacy were presented to the subjects was counterbalanced to reduce the likelihood of establishing a "carry-over" response bias. This random presentation, however, may have produced its own systematic order effect. To examine this possibility, a MANOVA was conducted to determine if any of the outcome measures differed significantly on the basis of presentation order. Although the omnibus MANOVA did not attain the preset alpha level of .05 (Wilks' lambda E (20,240) = 1.53, p = .07), the fact that it 97 approached statistical significance prompted an examination of the univariate protected E-tests. None of the four univariate E-tests were statistically significant at the .05 level (self-schema E (5,75) = 1.72, p = .14; mood state E (5.75) = 1.44, p = .22; self-efficacy E (5.75) = 0.40, p_ = .85: performance E (5,75) = 2.02, p_ = .09). It was therefore concluded that counterbalancing the presentation order of the outcome measures was successful in preventing response biases due to the order of presentation. Reliability ef Quteeme Measgres Reliability 9f self-sehemg instggments. Because the two instruments used as indices of self-schematicity in the present study (PPA(des) and PPA(imp)) were constructed by substantially modifying the Perceived Physical Ability (PPA) subscale of the Physical Self-Efficacy Scale (Ryckman, et al., 1982), it could not be assumed that the psychometric properties of the original instrument (Gayton, et al., 1986; McAuley & Gill, 1983; Ryckman, et al., 1982) were transferred to the modified versions. Therefore, two aspects of reliability -- stability and internal consistency -— were examined for each instrument. Stability of each instrument was assessed by calculating a test—retest reliability (correlation) coefficient between subjects' PPA(des) and PPAump) scores obtained during the in-class pre- experimental schematicity screening phase of the study and those obtained during the pre-manipulation assessments of the experimental phase of the study. Test-retest intervals ranged from 1 1 to 32 days with the mean interval being approximately three weeks in length (if = 21.7 days; sd = 6.3 days). With all 81 subjects included in the analysis, a test-retest reliability coefficient of .77 was obtained for the 98 PPA(des) and .84 for the PPAflmp)‘ The degree to which the two schematicity scales were internally consistent (measured a single, homogeneous domain) was assessed by computing Cronbach alpha reliability coefficients (Cronbach, 1951) for each scale. The obtained alpha reliability coefficients for the PPA(des) and PPAump) scales were .81 and .90, respectively. Both self-schema assessment instruments were, therefore, assumed to measure self-descriptions and importance placed on physical abilities with adequate stability and internal consistency. Reliability ef me Prefile ef Meeg State subseales. Estimates of alpha reliability (internal consistency) were computed for each of the six POMS subscales, as well as for the overall Total Mood Disturbance score which is a composite mood index derived by summing the six subscales (Vigor-Activity scale negatively weighted). Table 5 reveals that the reliability coefficients for four of the six POMS subscales as well as that for the Total Mood Disturbance score exceeded the .70 level set forth by Jensen (1978) as the generally acceptable standard for reliability estimates. The two subscales failing to meet this criterion were those purporting to assess the Confusion-Bewilderment (t = .59) and Anger-Hostility (t = .67) dimensions. P -M ul n 1.11 Mampulatten Qheek Although priming effects have been observed even when the primes have been presented subliminally and/ or when subjects are unable to recall the prime (e.g., Bargh, 1982; Bargh & Pietromonaco, 1982; Lombardi, et al., 1987), establishing that the manipulation conditions differed significantly in terms of their positive or negative 99 Table 5. Alpha Reliability (Internal Consistency) Coefficients for Six POMS Subscales and Total Mood Disturbance Scores. POMS Subscale Coefficient Alpha Tension-Anxiety .73 Depression-Dejection .86 Anger-Hostility .67 Vigor-Activity .90 Fatigue-Inertia .88 Confusion—Bewilderment .59 Total Mood Disturbance .87 100 valence would further increase confidence placed in the results of the study. Therefore, subjects were asked, as part of the exit questionnaire, to rate, on a 5-point scale, the degree to which they felt the coach depicted in the videotape was behaving in a positive or negative manner toward her team (1 = very negative toward her team; 3 = neutral toward her team; 5 = very positive toward her team). A oneway ANOVA revealed a significant omnibus E ratio (E (2.78) = 64.18; p < .001) indicating that perceived differences existed among the three manipulation groups relative to the positive / negative behaviors exhibited by the coach in the videotapes. Figure 4 graphically depicts the finding that planned orthogonal contrasts among the three manipulation groups revealed statistically significant differences among the three groups (positive condition i = 4.22, sd = 1.28; neutral condition if = 3.63, s51 = 1.04; negative condition it“ = 1.19, stt = 0.74). The significant differences among the three experimental groups indicate that the videotaped manipulations were successful in effectively depicting the coach exhibiting three distinct types of social interaction with her team. Specifically, subjects exposed to the positive coach perceived the videotaped behaviors as significantly more positive than did subjects who viewed either the neutral or negative coach videotape. Conversely, subjects exposed to the negative videotape perceived the coach's behaviors as significantly more negative than subjects in either the positive or neutral condition. Effeet ef Priming en Qateeme Variables The major purpose of the present study was to assess the efi'ect unobtrusive exposure to either a positive or negative social prime has on subsequent ratings of self-schema, self-efficacy, mood state and 101 motor performance. Descriptive statistics for the four post— manipulation outcome variables are presented in Table 6. t = 10.69, p <.001 ( ( Positive Neutral Negative Manipulation Manipulation Manipulation Condition Condition Condition (2 = 4.22, sd = 1.28) (r: = 3.63, sd = 1.04) (J? = 1.19, sd = 0.74) T M i t = 2.09, p < .05 t = 8.60, p < .001 Figure 4. Results of planned orthogonal contrasts among manipulation groups on perceived positive/ negative style of videotaped coach. The most appropriate method for assessing the overall effect of priming on the four outcome variables was through the use of a MANOVA formulated to test a doubly multivariate with repeated measures design (i.e., multiple dependent measures collected at multiple times). Results of this omnibus MANOVA indicated that although there were no significant main effects for either priming condition (Wilks' lambda E (8,150) = 0.91, p = .51) or time (Wilks' lambda E (4,75) = 0.94, p = .44) there was a significant interaction between priming and time (Wilks' lambda E (8,150) = 3.95, p < .001). Thus, there was an overall significant difference between subjects' pre- and post-manipulation outcome variables when examined as a function of the priming condition to which they had been exposed (i.e., a significant priming effect). 102 8.8. 8.8.. 8.8. 8.28 8.8. 8.8... 8.8. 288 888.288 28. 8.82 8.8. 882 E8. 8.8 8.8. 8.82 .888 8.2. 8.8 8.2. 8.8 8.2. 8.8 8.8. 8.8 .082 288. 5.8. 8.28 8.8. 8.2.8 8.8. 888 8.8. 888 «888 2.8. 288 8.82. 23.8 8.8. 2.8... 8.8. 8.88 88888 88. 8.82 88. 8.8 8.8. 8.82 8.8. 8.82 .6828 8.2. 8.8 8.2. 8.8 8.2. 8.8 8.2. 8.8 .082 88.888 8.8. 8.28 .25.. 8.82 8.2.. 888 8.8. 8.22 «82.8 62mm. 289..» Ammfih. mmémw 882m. 0069‘ fiado: Zfiom 88:28.82 22.8. 8.22 8.8. 2 2.82 8.8. 8.8 8.8. $.22 8888 88888 8.2. 2.8 8.2. 8.8 8.2. 8.8 88. 8.8 .082 3882 8.8. 888 8.8. 8.2a 8.8. 8.88 8.2.. 8.88 «888 8.8. 8.2... 8.8. 8.88 8.22. 8.8... 8.8. 8.2... 8858.8 8.8. 8.82 88. 8.22 8.8. 8.8 8.8. 21.2 .9828 88888 8.8. 88 8.2. 8.8 8.8. 8.8 88. 8.8 832 «>88 8.8. 8.88 8.8. 2.88 8.8. 8.88 8.8. 8.88 «888 Um M on M Um M um N 88.2. 5080 £080 £080 .882 «>882 «>88 828215 2888202 Gonwamgzbmom =4 .22 moamnflw 952.882 .0 038,—. 103 Although the results of the doubly multivariate with repeated measures MANOVA clearly indicate that differential exposure to the positive, negative or neutral prime resulted in significant pre- to post- manipulation differences on the outcome variables, univariate analyses, were required to explore the exact nature of these effects. A series of orthogonal contrasts between the positive, neutral and negative priming conditions were conducted comparing pre- to post- manipulation changes in each of the outcome variables. As depicted in Figure 5, when compared to the negative priming manipulation, exposure to the positive social prime resulted in significantly more positive self-schemata ratings (1; = 3.66: p < .001) and self-efficacy judgments (j; = 4.33: n < .001) as well as a general improvement in motor performance scores (1 = 2.97 ; p < .01). Of the four outcome variables measured, only mood state was unaffected by differential exposure to the positive or negative social prime. In addition. significant differences in self-schema ratings (1; = 2.33; p < .05) and self-efficacy judgments Lt = 2.54; p < .05) were observed between subjects exposed to the positive and neutral social prime. Comparable differences, however, did not exist between the negative and neutral priming conditions. Therefore, when taken as a whole, exposure to the positive social prime appears to have exerted a greater influence on self-referent cognitions than did the negative prime. Based on previous findings that schematics, relative to aschematics, are more successful in resisting counterschematic information (Markus, 1977; Markus, Crane & Siladi, 1978; Markus & Sentis, 1982; Sweeney & Morland. 1980), it was hypothesized 104 03029 080026 5000 5 808.2026 822 S 0.5 .28 282980 8582:9285 2200.502 B8580 gum 8. "a 8.2 ".2 8. "a .883 .8. um 8.2 ".2 8. vq .883 02:. H Um mofim n cw «as: n «8 Emma H cm 2 2mm H Um «93 u 00 mafia- u m 3.2 n M mwdm u m 28.3- n m .38 u M 9.2 u m 28:20:00 28:20:00 2056280 2022880 2056200 28:20:00 2050222802 22839202 22839202 202022982 2020392822 228229282 005mg 2 8.220 2 020%62 0.5%? 2 8.220 2 0.5%.92 8.v a .38 an 28¢ a 8.2. u H locum—Elnora." Managua—om 8. an 8.0".“ 2.12.8.2 ".2 8.12.8.2 "q 8. vq 8.8 ".2 52m H Um wmfi H Um mad H 00 mafia u cm «Wan u um «Q62 u on 2.0- um 2.0- "M 8.2- um 8.2- um 2.8-3." 8.0um 222980 2022880 2202:0200 2026280 2208200 202880 2020282282 20203982 28283982 nozflsnamz 22839202 20203982 0>2Mm02 8.2202 0>3Wmom 022mg 2 8.220 2 0>2Wom 2.12.8.2 uq 28.; .88 nu 338 8 105 (Research Hypothesis 5) that there would be a significant "prime x self-schematicity" interaction. Specifically, it was predicted that both positive and negative schematics would be most effective in resisting primes that were inconsistent with their prevailing self-schema for physical ability (i.e., positive schematics would efi'ectively resist the influence of the negative prime and negative schematics would effectively resist the influence of the positive prime). In contrast, due to the lack of a prevailing self-schema, aschematics should be equally susceptible to both forms of priming. To test this hypothesis, a 3 X 3 (priming manipulation by self-schematicity block) MANOVA was conducted with pre-post difi'erence scores on the four outcome variables serving as multiple dependent measures. Although there was a significant main effect for priming condition (E (8,138) = 3.87, p < .001), thereby supporting the previously described doubly multivariate findings, the results failed to support either a main effect for schematicity block (E (8,138) = 0.83, n = .57) or the prime by schema interaction hypothesis (E (16,211) = 0.79, p = .69). The effects of priming were, therefore, not mediated by the subjects' pre-existing self-schema for physical ability (i.e., positive, negative, aschematic). R l n hi Am 11 V bl The finding that self-schemata, self-efficacy and motor performance can be differentially primed by exposure to positive or negative social contexts served as an impetus to investigate the nature of the causal relationships existing among these variables. Three conditions must exist in order to establish causation between variables. First. there must be a statistical relationship (i.e., correlation) between the variables. Second, there must be temporal precedence (i.e., the 1 O6 "causal variable" must precede the "effected variable"). Finally, the error terms associated with the variables must not be correlated (i.e., there can exist no confounding variables). By specifying the temporal relationship, and assuming the non-correlation of error terms, path analysis enables the examination of the correlation between two variables (or multiple correlations among more than two variables) to establish proof of causation (Asher, 1983). To examine the causal relationships among priming and the outcome variables, a recursive (non-reciprocal) path analysis using ordinary regression techniques was conducted to test the model specified in the research hypotheses. Specifically, it was predicted that: (a) a positive or negative self-schema would be difi'erentially primed as a result of exposure to either the positive or negative manipulation (Research Hypothesis 1), (b) information upon which task-specific self-efficacy predictions are based would be selectively and differentially processed depending on the type of self-schema that had been invoked through the priming process (Research Hypothesis 2), and (c) due to a positive relationship between self-efficacy and performance, subjects in the negative priming condition would perform less well on a subsequent motor task relative to subjects assigned to the positive priming condition (Research Hypothesis 4). In addition, it was predicted that exposure to the priming condition would have a direct effect on mood state which would, in turn, impact on performance (Research Hypothesis 3). These hypothesized relationships among the variables are illustrated by the recursive model depicted in Figure 6 and the correlation matrix to be analyzed presented in Table 7. In path analytic terms, the priming variable (a 107 00382.5, 080080 _8 :0 00800 028820 28 @228 @888 0920:0328 002005093 8.2 02082000 50m .3 :09... All Fm J :0 000.6 08. 6 0006 :2. 88050 .02.. «Eon—om ooggouhom Al .200 Al .200 1/02. 82. 0800 \ 0002 «mm.» main 108 .8. v8: .00. v a .. oo. 2 000.888.2002 9. oo. 2 0800 0002 H N. - mm. 00. H 208020.200 fiwa {imm.l *QHfi. CO. H NEOSOMIH—Qm 2..er 9.- tam. 32‘. 00.2 0:82 A808 00200982 00800 88020 880200 085 GOAHNEHOMHOAH @002 IM—Om IM—Qw 00382.85 0800050 2 :0 00800 0080029 0002-05 28 mg M882 982.820.8800 .5 038B 1 09 dummy variable which was coded either "0" (negative prime) or "1" (positive prime) to represent priming condition membership) is said to be "exogenous" to the system in that it is not influenced by any of the other variables in the proposed model. On the other hand, the four outcome variables are considered "endogenous" because they are proposed to be the result of other variables in the model. The observed standardized path coefiicients (B), which serve as estimates of the magnitude of the causal linkages between the variables, are also provided for each predicted relationship. Finally, all unspecified sources of variation in each endogenous variable is estimated by a residual (error) term (st). Residual path coefficients are calculated using the equation: Rj= \/1-R‘:7 where R12 is the multiple correlation coefficient representing the amount of the total variance explained by the predictor variables. By examining Figure 6, it can be seen that, as predicted, there was a significant path between priming condition and physical ability self-schema ratings ([3 = .452, 1; = 3.66, p < .01). Specifically, exposure to the positive social prime significantly predicted positive changes in self-schema ratings. A second significant path existed between self-schema ratings and self-efficacy expectations ([3 = .409, t = 3.24, p < .01). Positive changes in pre-post self-schema ratings (after exposure to the social prime) significantly predicted positive changes in self-efficacy expectations (higher efficacy judgments). 110 However, Research Hypothesis 3, which predicted a significant causal relationship between priming condition and Total Mood Disturbance score and Research Hypothesis 4 which predicted a significant causal relationship between self-efficacy expectations and motor performance, were not supported by the path analysis. Because only hypothesized relationships among the variables are incorporated into the hypothesized model (in path analytic terms the model is said to be "overidentified"), it is possible to assess the overall quality of the model by comparing its "goodness of fit" to that of the complete (fully-identified) model (Figure 7). Using a formula suggested by Pedhazur (1982), a chi square goodness of fit test was calculated revealing a statistically significant difference between the fully identified and the predicted causal models (x2(5) = 18.90; p < .01; see Appendix M for the calculation of x2 ). This rejection of the null hypothesis (i.e., that there is no difference between the two models) indicates, according to Pedhazur, "that the model does not fit the data" (p. 617). It is worth noting that inspection of the fully-specified model (Figure 7), reveals significant paths between priming condition and self-schema ratings (B = .452: t = 3.66; n < .01), self-efficacy expectations ([3 = .414; 1; = 3.16; p_ < .01) and performance ([3 = .321; 1 = 2.00; p < .05). None of the other path coefficients in the full model attained the .05 level of statistical significance. gsnsral Discussion of Q1: Rssglts Findings in the present study support the contention that even brief and indirect exposures to seemingly irrelevant positive and negative social contexts are capable of exerting considerable influence 111 3 ; \ 0038280, 0880050 :8 Go 00800 008000.20 28 222.5 08088 09208028200 20008 :2 05 8.“ 028202000 582 .Iualh E i \.\i 00830020.— 080. All / N00. 208022.“ .200 / “:2 .. / All 0.2. \ _ 1.8:. + 002—0 6002 000.9 f 0880200 .200 ohm. boo. \ 000. All. .IVI: 0880 l l 2 on the activation of self-referent cognitions. Additionally, and of even more importance, was the unique finding that such positive and negative social priming can also have an effect on the execution of a subsequent motor performance task. Given the subtle nature of the priming exposure, the influence priming had on performance is noteworthy despite accounting for less than 15% of the total variance in performance (52 = .144). Although there is considerable theoretical support for these findings, this was the first empirical investigation to hypothesize and test the simultaneous effects of cognitive priming on physical ability self-schemata, self-efficacy expectations, mood states and motor performance. It was further hypothesized that priming would not only directly affect self-schema ratings but it would indirectly influence self-efficacy and performance. Although path analysis revealed that priming produced significant changes in self—schema ratings which, in turn, resulted in significant changes in self-efficacy judgments, performance was not related to self-efficacy (see Figure 6). By demonstrating that priming can influence self-efficacy expectations, however, this study confirms -- and thus demands further investigation of -- Bandura's (1986) generally overlooked position that none of the four major sources of self-efficacy he proposes (i.e., previous performance attainments, vicarious experience, verbal persuasion, assessment of physiological states) is inherently capable of generating efficacy judgments. As Bandura suggests, it is the W infgrmaflgn from each of these sources that conveys an efficacy expectation. A noteworthy finding in the present study is that priming -- by exposure to positive or negative social contexts -- appears to be 113 one factor capable of altering the manner in which this cognitive processing occurs. Given the robust nature of previous findings linking self-efficacy expectations with performance (See Feltz, 1992; 1988b) it is interesting to note that despite cognitive priming's significant effect on both self-efficacy expectations and motor performance, no relationship existed between self-efficacy expectations and performance. Although unexpected, a number of possible explanations exist for such a finding. Bandura (1986), for example. has identified several variables which can affect the strength of the relationship between self-efficacy judgment and performance. In the present study, at least three factors outlined by Bandura may have contributed, to some extent. to the lack of a positive relationship between self- efficacy and performance: (a) lack of consequences of misjudgment; (b) temporal disparities; and (c) obscure aims and performance ambiguities. Not only were there no negative consequences associated with subjects' misjudgments of their self-efficacy, but little or no motivation was provided for accurate efficacy judgments. In addition, temporal disparities between self-efficacy judgments and performance were inadvertently created by counterbalancing the sequence of instrument administration. For example, some subjects completed the self- efiicacy instrument immediately prior to performing the golf putting task whereas another subjects completed the mood states and / or the self-schema assessments in the interval between the efficacy ratings and performance. Partial support for this temporal explanation is provided by the fact that the omnibus MANOVA conducted to 114 determine if any of the outcome measures differed on the basis of presentation order approached (but did not attain) the preset alpha level of .05 (Wilks' lambda E (20,240) = 1.53, n = .07). Furthermore, although none of the four univariate F-tests conducted on the dependent variables were found to be statistically significant at the .05 level, the E value associated with performance again approached statistical significance E (5.75) = 2.02, p = .09). Finally, and perhaps most importantly, using pre- to post- manipulation change scores as dependent measures assumes that the only intervening variable between pre- and post-manipulation assessments is the experimental variable of interest (i.e., priming). Although this assumption may be justified when considering the self- schemata, mood state and performance variables, self-efficacy judgments present a unique problem. Subjects' pre-manipulation efficacy judgments were made prior to performing the task. Therefore, expectations were the made solely on the basis of verbal information provided by the experimenter. Post-manipulation efficacy expectations, on the other hand, were the result of both exposure to the experimental manipulation and previous personal experience (pre- manipulation performance) on the task. Considerable support for this explanation is provided by the fact that if only post-manipulation performance scores (as opposed to performance change scores) are examined, the expected positive correlation exists between self- efficacy and performance (r = .38; p < .01). Another unanticipated finding was that priming did not influence self-perception of mood states. Other investigators have reported similar non-significant effects of priming on mood states. Homstein 1 1 5 et al. (1975), for example, found no statistically significant differences in subjects' mood as measured by the Mood Adjective Check List after exposure to good or bad news broadcasts -- despite the changes they elicited in aggression (Study 1) and attitude toward others (Study 2). Similarly, Holloway, et a1. (1977) found no mood effects after administering priming manipulations consisting of good and bad social and nonsocial news stories. Although it may appear that the true relationship between priming and mood may have also been obscured by the unreliability of the POMS instrument (two subscales had internal consistency coefficients below .70), it must be remembered that individual POMS subscales were not used as dependent measures of mood state. Instead, overall Total Mood Disturbance (TMD) scores were derived by summing the six subscales and the reliability of TMD scores was a very respectable .87. Further indication that the lack of priming effects on mood was not due to unreliability in mood state measurement is provided by the fact that even correcting for attenuation due to unreliability (Ghiselli, Campbell & Zedeck, 1981) does not appreciably increase the correlation between priming conditions and mood states (uncorrected r = .19; corrected 1 = .21). A more feasible explanation for the lack of priming effects on mood may be gleaned from the results reported by Baldwin et al. (1990). In their study, affect was primed only when the social stimulus was a meaningful authority figure. Because the present investigation employed a manipulation which depicted an unknown and unidentified basketball coach interacting with her team, it is possible that she was not perceived by the subjects as a significant authority figure. 116 Schematicity blocks were constructed to take into account possible individual x priming condition interactions (Higgins, et al.. 1982; Martin, 1986). Despite statistically significant pre-manipulation differences between the schematicity groups, no schematicity x priming condition interactions were observed. The fact that exposure to either the positive or negative social prime had the same effect regardless of the subjects' prevailing self-schemata, does not support previous findings that schematics are more successful in resisting counterschematic information than aschematics (Markus, 1977; Markus, Crane & Siladi, 1978; Markus & Sentis, 1982; Sweeney & Morland, 1980). Because the previous studies did not employ a priming paradigm, one possible explanation for the divergent findings may involve the way priming stimulates increased cognitive elaboration during information processing. In their work on persuasion, Petty and Cacioppo (1986) have set forth a theoretical framework -- the elaboration likelihood model -- asserting that information can be processed either with considerable mental effort (central route) or in a relatively effortless fashion (peripheral route) depending on situation-specific cues and contexts. Central route processing generally involves thoughtful consideration of the message content whereas peripheral processing is accomplished through the use of available heuristics and mental shortcuts. It is possible, therefore, that exposure to either schematic or counterschematic primes serve as cues for further cognitive elaboration, and thus force a more critical examination of the message content relative to information which is presented without a prime. CHAPTER V SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS 51mm The present study examined the extent to which unobtrusive exposure to an irrelevant positive or negative social context was capable of producing differential (priming) effects on subsequent self- perceptions and motor performance. Specifically, female college students were exposed to the positive or negative communications of an athletic coach to determine the effect of cognitive social priming on self-schema, self-efficacy, mood states and performance on a golf- putting task. On the basis of previous theory and research it was hypothesized that: (a) a positive or negative self-schema would be differentially primed as a result of exposure to either the positive or negative manipulation, (b) information upon which task-specific self- efficacy predictions are based would be selectively and differentially processed depending on the type of self-schema that had been invoked through the priming process, and (c) due to a positive relationship between self-efficacy and performance, subjects in the positive priming condition would perform better on a subsequent motor task than those subjects assigned to the negative priming condition. Furthermore, it was predicted that exposure to the priming condition would have a direct effect on mood state which would, in turn, impact on motor performance. Using a two-stage data collection procedure, 1018 female undergraduates were initially screened to assess their physical ability self-schemata. After eliminating 505 subjects for responding in a socially desirable manner, the remaining 513 subjects were classified 117 I 18 as having either: (a) a positive self-schema for physical ability, (b) a negative self-schema for physical ability, (0) no self-schema for physical ability (aschematic), or ((1) none of the above self-schemata for physical ability. On the basis of their physical ability schematicity classification, a total of 81 subjects (27 positive schematics; 27 negative schematics; 27 aschematics) were recruited to participate in the experimental phase of the study. Prior to exposure to the experimental manipulation, each subject was assessed as to her self- schema for physical ability, current mood state, task-specific self- efficacy expectations and performance on a golf putting task. Subjects within each self-schematicity block were randomly assigned for unobtrusive exposure to a 5—minute videotape which depicted either a positive, negative or neutral social context. All social contexts were similar in that they portrayed a female basketball coach addressing her team. In the positive priming condition, the coach was shown encouraging and reinforcing positive aspects of the team's performance. In the negative priming videotape, however, the coach was seen criticizing and ridiculing her players for their poor performance. Finally, the neutral prime consisted of the coach giving the team emotionally neutral information about an upcoming road trip. After exposure to the experimental manipulation, each subject again completed written assessments related to her physical ability self- schema, current mood state and task-specific self-efficacy. In addition, each subject performed a second (post-manipulation) trial on the golf putting task. A multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) formulated to test a doubly multivariate with repeated measures design (i.e., multiple 119 dependent measures collected at multiple times) revealed that there was an overall statistically significant interaction between the priming condition to which subjects had been exposed and differences between pre— and post- manipulation scores on the dependent measures. Although no evidence was found to support the hypothesis that there would be a main effect of priming on mood state, subsequent univariate analyses (orthogonal contrasts) revealed that when compared to the negative social prime, exposure to the positive social prime resulted in significantly more positive self-schemata and self-efficacy judgments as well as a general improvement in motor performance scores. In addition, significant differences in self- schema ratings and self-efficacy judgments were observed between subjects exposed to the positive and neutral social prime. Because comparable differences did not exist between the negative and neutral priming conditions, it was concluded that the positive social prime exerted a greater influence on self-referent cognitions than did the negative prime. In addition, there was no evidence of a hypothesized priming x pre-existing physical self-schematicity interaction. Thus. the results of this study do not support the previous findings that schematics are more successful than aschematics in resisting counterschematic information (Markus, 1977 ; Markus, Crane 8r Siladi, 1978; Markus & Sentis, 1982; Sweeney & Morland, 1980). Finally, a causal model of the hypothesized relationships among the variables was tested against a fully-identified model and was found to not fit the data. 120 Conclusions Based on the findings, and within the limitations of the present study, the following conclusions are warranted: 1) 2) 3) When pre-existing self-efficacy expectations are controlled for by using pre- to post-manipulation change scores as a dependent measure, unobtrusive exposure to either a positive or negative prior social context produces a differential priming effect on subsequent self-efficacy expectations. Specifically, self-efficacy judgments become more positive after exposure to a positive social prime. When pre-existing self-schema ratings are controlled for by using pre- to post-manipulation change scores as a dependent measure, unobtrusive exposure to either a positive or negative prior social context produces a differential priming effect on subsequent self-schema ratings. Specifically, self-schema ratings become more positive after exposure to a positive social prime. When the level of pre—existing motor performance is controlled for by using pre- to post-manipulation change scores as a dependent measure, unobtrusive exposure to either a positive or negative prior social context produces a differential priming effect on subsequent motor performance. Specifically, motor performance is improved after exposure to a positive social prime. 121 4) A model that adequately explains the causal relationships between self-schemata, self-efficacy, mood states and motor performance has yet to be specified. Implicaflons for Practitioners and Suggestions for Future Research The findings of the present study hold practical implications for teachers and coaches as well as methodological implications for future research examining the effects of cognitive social priming. Although the impact a positive or negative coaching style can have on an athlete's self-perception and performance has long been a fertile topic for speculation among athletes, coaches and spectators, it has only recently gained the attention of the scientific community. Impeding scientific inquiry into the effects of coaching style has been the fact that much of the previous research has been conducted from an atheoretical perspective. Furthermore, interest in the area of coaching styles has tended to narrowly focus on the effects coaching behaviors (e.g., verbal rewards and punishments) have on the specific target(s) of those behaviors (e.g., the self-concept of the particular athlete(s) being praised or punished) -- as opposed to the broad manner in which a coach's public praise or criticism (such as that frequently witnessed during locker room "critiques") may affect the perceptions and/ or performances of seemingly uninvolved (i.e., non- targeted) observers (e.g., other team members) who are often forced to witness such behaviors. Because the emphasis has been on atheoretical investigations employing quasi-experimental research paradigms, even fundamental questions such as: "Is a negative coaching style more or less conducive to performance enhancement?" cannot be answered with any degree of certainty. 122 The present study hypothesized and tested relationships among three distinct psychological theories -- cognitive priming theory, self- schema theory and self-efficacy theory -- in a way that may shed light on cognitive mechanisms underlying the commonly held belief that relatively stable and predictable relationships exist between coaching style and athlete self-concept, confidence and motor performance. It was demonstrated that —- at least within a controlled laboratory setting -- it is possible to systematically alter the self-schema ratings, self- efficacy expectations and motor performances of uninvolved observers simply by exposing them to a coach interacting with others in either a positive or negative manner. In the immediate future, research is needed to replicate the preliminary findings of this study. If reproducible in the laboratory setting, the next step would be to embark on a path of programmatic research into the effects of positive and negative coaching/ teaching styles -- by elaborating, refining and subsequently validating and cross- validating the theoretical models through field experiments. One useful elaboration may be to design a study employing several sequential priming manipulations. For example, an AB design may be used to expose the same subject (each subject would act has his/her own control) to one type of prime (e.g., positive), then the other (e.g., negative), to assess the impact of each successive prime. Using this design would also allow more elaborate causal models to be hypothesized and tested in "waves" or "panels" (e.g., Feltz, 1982). Furthermore, although the present study demonstrated motor skill performance can be positively or negatively influenced by exposure to prior social contexts, a logical extension of this finding 1 23 would involve the examination of priming effects on motor skill eeguieition. In the meantime, practicing coaches (and teachers of all kinds) may do well to consider the way their behavior may be inadvertently influencing their athletes' and students' self-image, self- confidence and performance. FOOTNOTES 1 The four criteria used for inclusion in the W P h l i In x T rm are: (1) the frequency of the term's occurrence in the psychological literature, (2) the term's potential usefulness in providing access to a concept, (3) the term's relationship to or overlap with existing Thesaurus terminology, and (4) user feedback and need. (American Psychological Association, 1988). 2 The Qxferd English Qietienaiy credits Immanuel Kant as the originator of the term "schema" as follows: "In Kant: Any one of certain forms of rules of "productive imagination" through which the understanding is able to apply its "categories" to the manifold of sense- perception in the process of realizing knowledge or experience." 124 APPENDICES APPENDIX A Self—Descriptive Version of the Perceived Physical Ability Scale (PPA(des)) w Directione: Please circle the QNE number that best indicates the extent to which each of the following items describe you. PLEASE LE THE ER -- '1‘ MARK BE EN THE E . This does This __O_N T describes me describe me I have excellent reflexes. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 I am n_ot agile and graceful. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 My physique is rather strong. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 I can't run fast. I 2 3 4 5 6 7 I don't feel in control when I take tests involving physical 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 dexterity. I have poor muscle tone. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 I take little pride in my ability in sports. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 My speed has helped me out of some tight spots. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 I have a strong grip. l 2 3 4 5 6 7 Because of my agility, I have been able to do things which many others could not do 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 125 APPENDIX B Self-Importance Version of the Perceived Physical Ability Scale (PPA(imp)) APPENDIX B: Directione: Please circle the 0__NE number that best indicates the extent to which each of the following items are important to you. PLEASE QIR MTHE _ __ _ ER , This is This is very M very important to me important to me 1. Having excellent reflexes. l 2 3 4 5 6 7 2. Having agility and grace. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 3. Having a physique that is rather strong. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 4. Being able to run fast. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 5. Feeling in control when I take tests involving physical I 2 3 4 5 6 7 dexterity. 6. Having good muscle tone. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 7. Taking pride in my ability in sports. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8. Having enough speed to help me out of tight spots. I 2 3 4 5 6 7 9. Having a strong grip. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 10. Being able to do things which many others can not do. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 126 APPENDIX Shortened Version (Form C) of the Marlow-Crowne Social Desirability Scale Listed below are a number of statements conmrning personal attitudes and traits. Read each item and decide whether the statement is true or false as it pertains to you personally. Please circle either the word Ir_ue or liaise for each item below. 1. It is sometimes hard for me to go on with my work if I am not encouraged ................. True 2. I sometimes feel resentful when I don't get my way ....................................... True 3. On a few occasions, I have given up doing something because I thought too little of my ability ..... True 4. There have been times when I felt like rebelling against people in authority even though I knew they were right ................................... True 5. No matter who I'm talking to. I'm always a good listener .......................................... True 6. There have been occasions when I took advantage of someone .............................. True 7. I'm always willing to admit it When I make a mistake ......................................... True 8. I sometimes try to get even rather than forgive and forget ................................. True 9. I am always courteous, even to peOple who are disagreeable .................................. True 10. I have never been irked when people expressed ideas very different from my own .......... True 11. There have been times when I was quite jealous of the good fortune of others ................. True 12. I am sometimes irritated by people who ask favors of me .................................. True 13. I have never deliberately said something that hurt someone's feelings ....................... True 127 False False False False False False False False False False False APPENDIX D Demographic/ Background Questionnaire APPEND ° BACKGROUND UESTIONNAIRE NAME: How old are you? How do you describe yourself? Native American Black or African-American Mexican or Chicano Puerto Rican or other Latin American Oriental or Asian-American White or Caucasian OOOOOO What is your current academic class standing? 0 Freshman 0 Sophomore 0 Junior 0 Senior 0 Graduate Student What is your approximate college GPA? 3.50 - 4.00 3.00 - 3.49 2.50 - 2.99 2.00 - 2.49 1.50 - 1.99 Below 1.50 000000 What is your college major? In your entire life, on how many total occasions have you played golf (either miniature or regulation)? None at all 1 - 2 times 3 - 5 times 6 - 10 times 1 l - 20 times More than 20 times OOOOOO 128 In the past calander year (12 months), on how many occasions have you played golf (either miniature or regulation)? None at all 1 - 2 times 3 - 5 times 6 - 10 times 1 1 - 20 times More than 20 times OOOOOO Have you ever owned your own set of golf clubs? 0 No 0 Yes Have You LLV I Played in a W (for exmple. '* baseball, football, basketball, hockey, soccer, etc.) in which you had a designated coach(es)? 0 No 0 Yes How much experience would you say you have playing in competitive team sports with a coach or coaching staffs? (please circle one of the numbers below) 1 2 3 4 5 None A great deal 129 APPENDIX E Abbreviated Version of the Profile of Mood States Fatigued Annoyed Discouraged Resentful Nervous Miserable Cheerful Bitter Exhausted Anxious Helpless Weary Bewildered Furious Full of pep Worthless Forgetful Vigorous Uncertain about things Bushed NOT AT ALL DDDDDUDUUDDDUUUDDD DO A LITTLE DDDDDDDDDDUUDUUDDD DU 131 W! DUDUDUDUDUDDUUDDUD DD UITE A BIT UDUUDDUDDDUDDDDDUU [313 m DDUDDDUDDDUUDDUUUD DD Fatigued Annoyed Discouraged Resentful Nervous Miserable Cheerful Bitter Exhausted Anxious Helpless Weary Bewildered Furious Full of pep Worthless Forgetful Vigorous Uncertain about things Bushed W. DDDUDUDDDDDDDDUDDD DD AM UDUDDUDDDUDUDUDUDU DD 131 MODERATELY DDDDDDDDUDUUUDDUUU DU QUITE A BIT DDDUDDDDDUDDDUDDDD DU m DDDDDDDDDDDDDDUUDD DD APPENDIX F Alternative Word List for the Profile of Mood States Origin—hi POMS item Tense Angry Worn out Unhappy Lively Confused Peeved Sad Active On edge Grouchy Blue Energetic Hopeless Uneasy Restless Unable to concentrate Fatigued Annoyed Discouraged Resentful Nervous Miserable Cheerful APPENDIX F: First Alternative Under a strain Mad Tired Feeling 10W Spirited Not knowing what to do Ticked off Heart broken On the go Jittery Crabby Down in the dumps Enthusiatic Feeling things are impossible Uncomfortable Unable to relax something to do Easily distracted Petered out Bothered Wanting to give up Holding a grudge Worried In poor spirits In a good mood 132 mime—ME Stressed Aggravated Burned out Displeased Full of energy Baffled Miffed Sorrowful Keeping busy Touchy Grumm' Down Peppy Having no alternatives 111 at ease In need of Unable to focus Run down Bugged Dampened spirit Harboring Uptight Woeful Happy mm POMS item Bitter Exhausted Anxious Helpless Weary Bewildered Furious Full of pep Worthless Forgetful Vigorous Uncertain about things Bushed First Alternative Sore Completely spent Concerned Unable to act Drained Puzzled Enraged Ready to go Useless Unable to remember Powerful Up in the air Pooped out 133 Second Alternative Hateful Unable to continue Fearful Vulnerable Beat Dumbstruck Furning Full of gusto Valueless Absent-minded Potent Doubtful All done in APPENDIX G Golf Putting-Specific Self-Efficacy Scale APPENDIX W How confident are you that you will be able to hit angst 1 of your 20 golf shots within 12 inches of the cross? 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 I'm certain I'm Mainly certain I'm certain l 9.13 do lcando l em do this this this How confident are you that you will be able to hit afleast 2 of your 20 golf shots within 12 inches of the cross? 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 I'm certain I'm mm certain I'm certain I an} do lcando I m do this this this How confident are you that you will be able to hit Mi 3 of your 20 golf shots within 12 inches of the cross? 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 I'm certain l'm mgfiemtely certain I'm certain I 93113 do lcando I an do this this this How confident are you that you will be able to hit eueest 4 of your 20 golf shots within 12 inches of the cross? 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 I'm certain I'm Misty certain I'm certain l 93:13 do lcando l ean do this this this How confident are you that you will be able to hit atleest 5 of your 20 golf shots within 12 inches of the cross? 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 I‘m certain I'm Mate)! certain I‘m certain I an] do lcando l m do this this this How confident are you that you will be able to hit etjeaet 6 of your 20 golf shots within 12 inches of the cross? 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 I'm certain l'm mmemely certain I'm certain I 93113 do lcando | ean do this this this How confident are you that you will be able to hit eLLeeet 7 of your 20 golf shots within 12 inches of the cross? 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 I'm certain I'm my certain I'm certain I emit do lcando I ran do this this this How confident are you that you will be able to hit angst 8 of your 20 golf shots within 12 inches of the cross? 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 3 9 10 I'm certain l'm medemjely certain I'm certain l 93:13 do lcando l all do this this this 134 10. 11. 12. 13. 14. 15. 16. How confident are you that you will be able to hit Mt 9 of your 20 golf shots within 12 inches of the cross? 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 I'm certain I’m my! certain I'm certain I m do lcando I can do this this this How confident are you that you will be able to hit Wt 10 of your 20 golf shots within 12 inches of the cross? 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 I'm certain I'm moderately certain I'm certain I 93113 do lcando I can do this this this How confident are you that you will be able to hit angst 11 of your 20 golf shots within 12 inches of the cross? 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 I'm certain I’m moderately certain I'm certain I gan'j do lcando I can do this this this How confident are you that you will be able to hit aghast 12 of your 20 golf shots within 12 inches of the cross? 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 I'm certain I'm mggflajgly certain I'm certain I gang do lcando I can do this this this How confident are you that you will be able to hit Mt 13 of your 20 golf shots within 12 inches of the cross? 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 I'm certain I'm My certain I'm certain I 93113 do lcando I can do this this this How confident are you that you will be able to hit Wt 14 of your 20 golf shots within 12 inches of the cross? 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 I'm certain I'm mm certain I'm certain I can} do lcando I an do this this this How confident are you that you will be able to hit aflgast 15 of your 20 golf shots within 12 inches of the cross? 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 I'm certain I‘m 1135519133911 certain I'm certain I 5333 do lcando l 9311 do this this this How confident are you that you will be able to hit Wt 16 of your 20 golf shots within 12 inches of the cross? 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 I'm certain I'm 03551912391! certain I'm certain I 93113 do lcando I can do this this this 135 17. 18. 19. 20. How confident are you that you will be able to hit angst 17 of your 20 golf shots within 12 inches of the cross? 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 I'm certain I'm moderately certain I'm certain l 9303 do lcando l m do this this this How confident are you that you will be able to hit gumt 18 of your 20 golf shots wimin 12 inches of the cross? 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 I'm certain I'm mm certain I'm certain I 93:13 do lcando I can do this this this How confident are you that you will be able to hit angst 19 of your 20 golf shots within 12 inches of the cross? 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 I'm certain I'm mm certain I'm certain I 93113 do lcando I can do this this this How confident are you that you will be able to hit all of your 20 golf shots within12 inches of the cross? 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 I'm certain I'm my certain I'm certain I 9393 do lcando I can do this this this 136 APPENDIX H Transcripts of Manipulation Videotapes APPENDIX H Prg-Maninuktign Tape (presented in a straight-forward, but pleasant lecturing style). Certainly, we want to have some sense of historical development. Maybe not to the degree that we did with the field at large, but certainly you want to pay attention to that. We also want to pay attention -— I'm going to drop down to the bottom -- to some of the issues and problems that are inherent to each subdiscipline and you will see some themes emerge from one subdiscipline to the other. And these are some of the debates and discussions that we find in the field at large. Also, we will want to pay attention to the role in modern society -- thus trying to take the study of a subdiscipline and make it practical. And, to a lesser degree, you'll want to be aware of the other categories. And Seidentop does a nice job of presenting the subdisciplines in this fashion. So as you read those chapters and as I present the material, I will try to adhere to this outline and you should try to pull those points forward from your reading. The first subdiscipline we're going to discuss is the one that came into play the earliest in our field and that is exercise physiology. If we think of the definition of physiology -- and you don't need to write this down, it's in the book -- physiology is the study of the functioning of plants and animals and of the activities by which life is maintained and reproduced including the functioning of, and inter-relationships among cells, tissues, organs and systems such as nervous, circulatory and respiratory. Exercise physiology is the study of those activities in that definition, under the stress of exercise -- under the stress of exercise. One of the early leaders in the field of exercise physiology was Dr. George Fitts. In fact, Dr. George is given the title of the "father of exercise physiology." And exercise physiology had its origins in the early 20th century when there was this debate about the systems -- the gymnastic systems -- which one's better? Which one is the "right" system? We should have noted that Fitts was the keynote speaker at the AAPE conference and also to his credit, he developed the first ex. phys. lab and that was at Harvard. Now as we consider exercise physiology -- its role in modern society is rather well-developed. Exercise physiology is probably the most widely known subdiscipline. Part of that is due to the explosion of interest in the adult population. Where are the greatest number of active participants coming from as far as the "fitness craze?" -- In the adult population. More fitness centers are booming. The fitness equipment business is raging, almost out of control -- there's a new shoe every week. And the clothes ~~ you've got to look it -- you've got to have the "look" if you're going to be in fitness. Videotapes, cable T.V., books -- go out to the bookstore sometime, out at the mall -- look at the shelves of fitness books. So, one of the roles in modern society is to debunk myths. 137 That's one of the roles of exercise physiology. Certainly another role, as well as debunking myths, is to provide information to the public -- particularly to the layperson —- to the average person. We know there are enough journals out there -- technical journals -- but that information has to become synthesized so it becomes usable by the average person. So, two roles in modern society. What are the principal areas of specialization? Well, there are two traditional areas. One is the utilization of oxygen in the C.V. system and that's particularly with aerobic activities -- V02 max, oxygen uptake. And the second area of specialization has been that of metabolic responses to exercise and training. As we might expect, given this area is growing, two new areas -- one that you're prolbably very familiar with -- cardiac rehab. and a new flourishing area is that of exercise biochemistry. Really trying to move to the micro—level of analysis in this area. When we look at the field and we look at exercise physiology, you can look at it from two perspectives -- one, from this new idea of "wellness." A second perspective would be performance improvement or performance enhancement. Exercise physiology has a pretty broad umbrella right now. Some people come through athletic training and work under the auspices of exercise physiologists. Certainly we recognize the adult fitness being under there —- the athletic training, strength training. And the anticipation is that this area is going to continue to grow. So in some circles there is this urge to become separate -- that we are big enough to support ourselves —— that we don't need to be under the umbrella. A blanket statement as to the roles of professionals. . . if you work at the university -- and many exercise physiologists do -- what are they expected to do at the university level -— and this will be true throughout our discussion of subdisciplines. Teach. Research. Advise. And there may -- with exercise physiologists -- be clinical work. That's not true with every subdiscipline. But these will be the three principal roles of professionals at the university level: Teach, conduct research and advise students. What are the employment opportunities for someone in exercise physiology? What do you think? We talked about career opportunities last week. . . good, bad? If it's the largest growing area what do you think? Pretty good. There are more and more adult fitness centers opening. Corporations are beginning to open more and more corporate fitness programs. There are opportunities in hospitals, certainly in athletic programs. And because of its popularity, there is greater pressure on universities to have a full staff of exercise physiologists to meet the demand of the students. What is the education and /or professional preparation that is required? What can you do with a bachelors degree? Nothing. What can you do with a master's degree in exercise physiology? Are there any possibilities? Yeah, there are. With a master's degree you could get a position as an exercise technician working in cardiac rehab or working in corporate fitness. If you want to go to the university what do you have to have? Got to 138 have a Ph.D. and that's going to become more and more prevalent at all levels. It used to be at some of the smaller institutions, a master's degree would gain you entry. But almost from top to bottom now, as far as size, a Ph.D. is going to be required. What are some of the issues and problems that face exercise physiologists? Well, one is going to be -- and you'll see this argument throughout -- is the issue of basic versus applied research. Developing the theoretical versus developing the practical. Which way we going to go? There is also some debate about being too narrow and then there is the other side of it -- being too broad. Being too narrow -- going to the cellular level and even below that, as far as understanding. Well what's the utility of, say, exercise biochemistry at the cellular level, to a coach? It's beyond them. It's just not relevant. So one of the issues is that many people are critiquing saying "Look, you're too narrow. You're going so microscopic that the information you're producing is useless -- it just fills the library shelves." And others say: "Well, when we try to broaden out, we can't really hone in on an understanding." 139 Negative Manipulation Tape (presented in a sarcastic, critical manner) All right you guys, I don't think I have to tell you this but you guys are doing a piss-poor job of shooting your free throws. I don't want to see any more of this so I'm going to sit here and for the umpteenth time I'm going to explain something to you I have already gone over probably a million times. We are in our sixteenth game this season. That means you've probably heard the same thing sixteen times already so I would think that it would start to sink into your heads after a while -- but NO, NO -- what do you do? You go out there, you get up to the free-throw line, you do whatever you want, you don't follow directions. I want to see you guys start following directions! First of all, remember, the word, the F word -- fundamentals. How many times have I told you, we can make our free throws if we practice the fundamentals. We've been practicing them again and again and again in practice but you get out there in the game and you keep on blowing it. I don't want to see this anymore. Now, the first thing I want you to do when you get up to the free-throw line -- and I want you to do it -- I don't want you to say "but coach, but coach" I don't want to hear that -- just do it! Now the first thing is you're going to have to get up there and relax. You guys are getting up there and you're all jittery -- I don't know what your problem is but you got to just get up there and relax. Start by taking a deep breath, get up to the line and bounce the ball a few times, do whatever it takes -- I don't care as long as you can get the ball in the basket! The second thing I want you to do is concentrate more. Shooting free throws is 90% mental. The way you guys are, you look like you have mental illness rather than you're concentrating and it's 90% mental. The third thing is I want you to get your body in the correct position. Most of you guys are not squaring up to the basket. Some of you are over here, some of you are over there. I want you to be squared up to the basket. I want you to get your index finger on the air hole. How many times have I told you that if you get the proper hand to ball relationship you're going to have an easier time making the basket? Get yourselves squared up. I don't want to see your toes pointing to the left, I don't want to see them pointing to the right. I just want them pointing directly at the basket. The other thing you guys are not doing -- and I've told you a million times -- if I've told you once I've told you a million times, you're not shooting at the front of the rim. I want to see you guys sighting in on the basket, I want to see you sighting in on the front of the basket. Instead, you guys are just banging your shots off the back of the rim or the backboard. I might as well stick a little rubber bumper on the back of the rim because you're never going to make it any other way -- you're just banging them in off the backboard. I want to see you shoot for the front of the rim. The other thing is that you guys aren't using any backspin. When it comes off your hand you have to get a 140 nice soft touch, you have to get some backspin on it so you have to have your fingertips pushing the ball so you get some backspin. I don't see you guys doing that! In fact, you might as well be standing up there shooting "granny" (underhanded) shots for all I care because you're not going to get any backspin on the ball any other way. The other thing I want you to do is follow-through. And I'm not just talking to the shooter -- I'm now talking to the whole team. First of all, for the shooter, get your finger over the air hole, I want to see your elbow lined up with the basket, I want to see the ball come off your fingertips and then I want to see your wrist look like a "goose- neck" when you get done. Not a "turkey-neck" -- you guys are shooting so horrible that I should just call your's a turkey-neck. As long as we're talking about follow—through, what I want the rest of the team to be doing -- I don't want the rest of you to be lollygaging around out there! You guys are just standing out there with your hands on your hips, you don't have your hands up while you're waiting for the rebound. You guys aren't even getting ready. While the shooter is trying to follow-through, what I want the rest of you guys to be doing is to think about what you're going to do to box out that woman next to you. How are you going to get in front of that person? GET IN FRONT OF THAT PERSON! You guys are not doing a good job of boxing the other team out. It's just a simple matter of stepping in front of the person -- but NO, what are you guys doing? "Oh, I'm too tired, I have my hands on my hips, oh gee, I'm really tired." I DON‘T WANT TO SEE ANY MORE OF THAT! When the ball is released, I want you guys to be ready. I want you to have your hands up, I want you to get ready to step out in front of the other person and get the other team boxed out. We're just not doing what we've practiced over and over again. These are fundamentals. You should have learned these things in the second grade! GIVE ME A BREAK! Finally, another thing I want to say about free throws -- and then we'll go on to talk about the rest of all the other things you're doing wrong -- which means we may be in the locker room for a whole hour -- you're doing so many things wrong. The other thing I want to tell you is that you've played a really poor first half -- just a really poor first half. I can't tell you how disappointed I am in that. You guys just have not done a good job at all! You guys are embarrassing me, you guys are embarrassing yourselves, you guys are embarrassing the whole school. If you guys want to get on the bus with your head hanging because you've been such an embarrassment to everyone ~— including yourselves -- fine, go ahead and be that way. But I expect you to at least do something to make the school proud, at least do something to make me proud because I'm sick and tired of seeing you guys just lollygaging around out there. The other thing I wanted to point out to you is that on offense -- we might as well come up with another name for it -- because we're certainly not making it look as if it's supposed to be offense. We're certainly not making it look as if we're attacking the other team. We're certainly not making it look like we're even trying to make a 141 basket! So what I want to see is this person who is playing point, I want to see some action, I want to see some movement out on the floor. I'm not seeing anything that looks like movement out there. You guys are just standing still. You're standing back on your heels. When the point guard is passing you the ball, you're not going to meet the ball, instead, you're just standing there waiting for it to come to you. WAITING FOR IT TO COME TO YOU! We're not playing some kind of slow-motion game here. Basketball is a game of action -- you're supposed to be moving! I want to see the point, if you throw it over on the strong side, I want to see you cut through. Now the other thing is, this guard over here on the weak side, I want you to move out to protect against the fast break! Were getting killed on r the fast break! They are fast breaking us to death because you're not moving out here and you're not getting ready to play defense. So I want to see this person slide over and get to move out. And, by the way, this person at the free-throw line, yeah, sure, we call you high post but it not because you're supposed to stand there like a stick in the mud, alright? Instead, I want you sliding down, sliding down and be ready for a pass or a rebound. Alright, now, the other thing I want to talk about is on our press. The thing we're doing wrong -- the first thing were doing wrong -- we're doing a lot of things wrong on our press. I just don't know where your heads have been at for the last sixteen games, I just really don't. But the first thing I want see on our press is I want to see, when the in-bounds pass comes in, I do not want to see that person dribble more than three times. I want to see that dribble stopped! Because we are not going to be able to stop them unless we get that person to pick the ball up and pass it from there. So I want to see that woman's dribble stopped. I do not want to see that person dribbling down -- or halfway down the court» like I've seen in the past. Now, I probably have a lot of other things to say to you but I guess you guys can just sit here and think about the things I've been telling you. 142 Positive Manipulation Tape (presented in an encouraging, positive manner) Hey, you guys, you know were having a little trouble out there on the free—throw line. You know I've seen you in practice and every time we're in practice you guys are great free-throw shooters and I think we can really help ourselves in this game. We're not down by too much so if we just do a little better job on our free throws I think we'll be right back in the game and we'll be able to win this game. I've told you some of these things before, and I'm not telling them to you to make you nervous or anything like that -- I just want to remind you of them. Just try to think like we're in practice. Because I've seen you guys in practice -- one right after the other you get up there and you just swish, swish, swish, swish -- you know I've seen it again and again so I know you guys can do a really good job on this. Let me just remind you about some of the things that maybe you're just forgetting a little bit while you're up there. The first thing is that I think you guys are just a little too tense and you just need to relax. So when you get up to the free-throw line, dribble the ball a few times, flex your knees a little bit, don't be so tense while you're up there, take a deep breath just before you get ready to shoot and then let the ball go when you feel relaxed. Now, sure, the refs standing there counting 10 seconds, OK, but 10 seconds is a long time and you can take the whole 10 seconds so don't let anybody pressure you or make you feel like you have to just jump up there and shoot it, and don't be quite so tense. The other thing I want you to do -- and it's related to not being so tense -- is you do need to concentrate just a little bit more. Like I've seen you in practice and you guys have really good concentration when you're in practice. Maybe it's just the crowd that's just throwing you off a little bit, I'm not really sure. But you guys have done this again and again in practice so let's just think about: "OK. I can do this, I've done this in practice, I'm just going to do this just like I did it in practice. Because shooting and making a free throw is 90% mental -- alright? So just forget about everything else and just think about it like it's in practice. The other thing I want you to do is to remember that I want you guys to square-up to the basket. I noticed that some of you guys, when you're out there, there were a couple people off to the right-hand side, and there were a couple people off to the left-hand side. Instead, I want to see your toe up to the free—throw line, pointing at the basket and I just want to see you squared-up to the basket. I want to see that elbow square in there ~- OK? So make sure that your body is in the right position -- just like in practice -- we've done it hundreds of times in practice and you guys have done a good job. I've even make you guys run suicides and your tongues have been hanging out in practice and you've still gone up there an make a lot of free throws so I know that you guys can do this. The other thing I want you to remember -- and it's something else we've talked about in practice -- make sure that 143 you are shooting at the front of the rim -- not the back of the rim, O.K.? Make sure you're shooting at the front of the rim. Because the other thing I want you to do is when you're shooting, put a little more backspin on the ball -- a little more backspin. Make sure it's got that nice soft shooter's "touch." That way we'll get a lot of swishers. Let's see how many swishers we can get O.K.? And I know you guys can do it because again, I've seen you guys ripping the nets during practice and it's just that right now we just somehow got a little bit off in this game. So shoot for the front of the rim and when you get that backspin then, the ball is going to be more likely to bounce in because it has that backspin on it. The other thing that you need to remember, O.K.? and I've seen you guys do this again in practice, so I know that you know this. I want you to remember that when you release the ball it's wrist action. A lot of it's wrist action. It's in the knees, it's in the legs and it's wrist action. So what I want to see is some follow-through with your wrist, O.K.? I want to see your wrist look like a "goose-neck." Now, I know you've probably heard that since second grade and it's just something that you have to keep in mind -- I know it's already in your mind -- it's just something you have to keep it a little more in the front of your mind. The other thing is -- O.K., we didn't have a very good first half. So let's just admit it -- we didn't have a very good first half. But that doesn't mean we're going to have a bad second half. In fact, I have seen you guys come from behind in the past -- I have seen you guys pull out a victory. So let's just pull together, let's just have a little bit of teamwork and let's just try to remember the fundamentals that we've learned in practice along the way. The other thing I wanted to mention about free throws -- and I wanted to talk to not just the shooter, but the rest of the team too. I want to see you guys boxing out the other players a little more. We practiced that a lot in practice and you guys have always done a good job. I mean, you guys have always been able to keep the scrimmage team out. You're always able to step out in front of them and keep them out of there. You've kept them out every time in practice and these guys aren't any different. They aren't any better than you so let's just get out there and make sure were boxing out our players. The other thing I'd like to see a little more of is -- I'm not seeing as much action as I'd like to see out on the floor. I'd like to see more movement on the floor. I'd like to see some real nice crisp passes just like we've been doing in practice. We've had that medicine ball and our arms get really heavy but you guys still come right back and you just snap the pass, O.K.? So I've seen that lots of times and I know that you can do it. So what I'd like to see is the person who's playing point -- I want to see a nice crisp pass over here -- say we go on the strong side -- and then I want to see some motion through here. Now I've seen a few times you've been running this play correctly, but every now and again we kind of just forget our positions so I just want to see you guys remember to do it every time -- not just a few times. So I want to see this person cut through, and over here, on the weak side, don't 144 forget to slide back out because we don't want to get killed on the fast break or anything, so don't forget to slide back out. And I really think we need to just forget about the first half, O.K.? The first half is the first half and we're not very far behind and I know we can catch these guys and I know we can catch up and I know that you guys can do it. So let's just forget about the first half and concentrate on some of these fundamentals that we've learned and hey, let's work hard. As long as we work hard, that's all we can ask. Then, when we get on the bus we can hold our heads up. 145 Neutral Manipulation Tape (presented in a straightforward, pleasant manner) Hey you guys, before you leave practice for today, I have to give you some extra information about our tournament that we are going to be in over the weekend. Now the procedures are a little bit different from what we usually do for a game because we don't usually stay overnight -- we just usually take a bus and come back after the game. This time, we will be playing on both Friday and Saturday and we're not coming back between games. So, I want to give you a little bit of information about the trip. I know that this is the first overnight trip some of you have ever made with the team. First of all, as you guys know, we're going to Grand Rapids. It's approximately about an hour or an hour and fifteen minutes from here so that's how long the bus ride will be. Now the one thing that you need to know for sure is that you guys are not responsible for paying for your hotel. You guys are not responsible for paying for breakfast or meals or anything like that -- all the necessities will be taken care of for you. You might, however, want to bring just a little bit of spending money along with you because, who knows, you might want to buy some little thing like a t-shirt in the hotel souvenir shop or something like that. Or you might want to have just a little bit of spending money, but, I really wouldn't recommend bringing along very much money because there's always a chance that it could get stolen or it could get lost or something like that. So, I don't know, you might want to bring $10.00 or so, but I wouldn't bring any more than that. After all, between the bus ride to and from Grand Rapids and the games we play, we won't have a lot of time for shopping. Again, you're not responsible for paying for the hotel, you're not responsible for your meals or anything like that. That will all be provided for you. And I think it should be a pretty good time all-in-all. Now, we will be playing in two games there. We will be playing on Friday night at 7 o'clock so the bus is going to leave here -- we want to go, get checked into our hotel, get all these things done, and then go over to the gym for the tournament -- so the bus is going to be leaving here almost directly after school at 3:30. So your folks should bring you back to school -- if you're going home after school to maybe pick up your duffel bag or whatever -- but you need to be back at the school at 3:15. We're going to get on the bus at 3:30, we're going to head over there, as soon as we get there we're going to check into the hotel and we'll grab something to eat. Now we're going to eat pretty light because we have the game at 7 o'clock. So we don't want to be eating a big meal or anything like that. So we're going to eat kind of light and then we got this game at 7:00. Now, as you know, if we win that game we will be playing in the championship game the next day. If we don't win the game, we will be playing in the consolation game the next day, O.K.? So, if your folks are interested in coming, there are two games they can come to -- Friday at 7 o'clock and one of the 146 two games on Saturday. Now the exact time of the Saturday game is obviously going to depend on whether we win or lose on Friday night. We are scheduled to stay for the championship game anyway on Saturday. If we are not in that game, we'll just stay on and watch the game anyway. Now, what we're going to do is we're going to divide up -- and I've got a list -- I've put people in alphabetical order and I've got a list of who's going to be staying in what rooms together with each other. We're not bringing -- there's just myself and the junior varsity coach that will be coming along and there's only the 12 of you so we pretty much figure that you guys are adults and you guys can handle this and stuff. We'll be just down the hall, so if your folks are concerned about that, we'll be just down the hallway. We'll be checking on you guys periodically. We're going to have a little bit of a curfew -- I know you guys don't really like that but were going to check to make sure you're back in your room at 1 1:30, O.K.? Because we're going to have to be ready for the game the next day. So you can tell your folks we'll be checking on you so everything will be cool that way. Now on Saturday, after the game, what we'll do is go back to the hotel and you guys can take a shower there if you want and we'll leave from there. We'll be back at the school then -- we should be back at the school by 8 pm. at the latest. So your folks can pick you back up at the school at 8 o'clock. So, I think this will be a good time and these are the pieces of information that you need. Can you guys think of any other questions that you might have? O.K., well, if you need any more information, or if your folks need any more information --— oh, by the way, I guess the other piece of information you need is that I will provide your folks with the phone number for the hotel and so forth. If they need any additional information, please have them call me. 147 w Description of Coaching Behavior Assessment System (CBAS) APPENDIX 1: OACHING BEHAVI R A E MENT Y EM BAS §CALES QSED IN THE PRESENT STUDY. A Reacp’gns tp Mist_a_kes: 1. Mistake-Contingent Encouragement. Encouragement of a player by a coach following a player's mistake. 2. Mist e- n ' n T chnic In tru tion. Telling or showing a player who has made a mistake how to make the play correctly. 3. Punishment. A negative response by the coach following an undesirable behavior. May be verbal or nonverbal. 4. Punitive. When mistake-contingent technical instruction and punishment occur in the same communication. B Game Related Spontaneous Behavigrs: 1. General Technical Instruction. A communication that provides instruction relevant to techniques and strategies of the sport in question. 2. General Encouragement. Encouragement that does not immediately follow a mistake. 3. Organization. Behavior directed at administrative organization. 148 APPENDIX J Manipulation Check Questionnaire APPENDIX J: EXIT U TIONNAIRE The following questions are to see what kinds of things caught your attention during the testing session. 1. What color was the box (on the tape) you were shooting at? 2. During your first "rest period", the experimenter was viewing a videotape. What was the tape about? 3. After you shot your first set of putts, the experimenter was viewing a different videotape. What was on this tape? 4. How many putts did you shoot in the total study? 5. What was the major pugpose of the experiment in which you just participated? 6. Please judge your performance on your first set of 20 golf putts. 1 2 3 4 5 I performed worse I performed about as I performed better than I thought I would well as I thought I would than I thought I would 7. Please judge your performance on your ficond set of 20 golf putts. 1 2 3 4 5 I performed m I performed about as I performed Mgr than I thought I would well as I thought I would than I thought I would 149 red X during your first set of 20? How many putts did you hit so they stopped directly on the 9. How many putts did you hit so they stopped directly on the red X during your second set of 20? 10. What color was the biofeedback dot when it was placed on your hand? 1 1. Watching the videotape of the coach while I was resting: 1 2 3 4 Helped my Had no affect on performance my performance 12. Do you feel the coach on the videotape was: 1 2 3 4 Very Negative Neutral Toward Her Team Toward Her Team 150 5 Hurt my performance 5 Very Positive Toward Her Team APPENDIX K Informed Consent Form APPENDIX K EEQMD QQNSEN T EQRM Michigan State University Dewent of Phfiical Education and Exercise Science Prin Inv r: Richard R. Albrecht, M.A. Doctoral Candidate / Research Assistant Dept. of Physical Education and Exercise Science Telephone: 353-9400 (A-212 East Fee Hall) MyAdvism: Deborah L. Feltz, Ph.D. Chairperson and Professor Dept. of Physical Education and Exercise Science Telephone: 355-4732 (138 I.M. Sports-Circle) I, . hereby agree to participate as a volunteer in the study of college student performance on a golf putting task. The purpose of this study is to collect normative data regarding this motor task and conduct a preliminary investigation into the way feelings and perceptions affect performance. The results of the study will be used to address previously neglected areas in the sport performance literature. Data will be collected by the principal investigator. The session will take approximately 45-50 minutes and be held in room E-4, East Fee Hall on the M.S.U. campus. I will be tested individually and will: (a) sit quietly for about 5 minutes; (b) complete three short questionnaires regarding my current feelings and perceptions about various physical activities and my confidence in performing these activities, and (c) attempt 20 golf putts. None of the information collected will be of a "sensitive" nature. In addition, the researchers have taken steps to protect the confidentiality of all individuals participating in the study. No one except the principal investigator will have access to the my answers and/or performance results. In reporting the results of this study, only summary data will be reported and no individual(s) will ever be identified in any way. My participation in this study is strictly voluntary. If I choose not to participate, there will be no penalty. If I do participate, I may choose to omit questionnaire items I do not wish to answer or I may discontinue my participation in the study at any time. If I complete the study, I will receive 2 half-hour "participation in psychology research" credits. I have read the above and I understand its contents and I freely agree to participate in this golfing study. I acknowledge that I am 18 years of age or older. Signature Date 151 Ilia. APPENDIX L BIODOT Color Reference Chart APPENDIX L BIODOTS Violet 94.6°F. - Very relaxed Blue 93.6°F. - Calm Turquoise 92.6°F. - Relaxing Green 91.6°F. - Involved (nmI) Yellow 90.6°F. - Unsettled Amber 89.6°F. - Tense Color approximations and general'lnterpretations of stress: Black 87°F. - Very Tense 152 APPENDIX M Calculations for x2 "goodness of fit" test between hypothesized and fully—identified causal models APPENDIX M The following formula for testing a hypothesized (overidentified) causal model against the fully-identified model is taken from Pedhazur (1982; pp. 617-628). It is first necessary to calculate 32m which is the generalized squared multiple correlation. For the fully recursive model: 32m = 1 - (1- 321) (1- 322) . . . (1- 32p) Where: 321 = the ordinary squared multiple correlation coefficient of 1th equation. A statistic similar to 32m is then calculated for the overidentified model (M): M = 1 - (1- R21) (1- R22) . . . (1- 3%) To quote Pedhazur (1982): M is calculated in the same manner as is 32m, except that some or all of the 325 are based on a model in which some of the paths have been deleted, whereas the 32s [used in the calculation of 32m] are based on a fully recursive model. Therefore, M can take values between zero and 32m. When the fit of an overidentified model is perfect (i.e., when 3 is exactly reporduced), 32m, = M. The smaller M is in relation to 32m the poorer the fit of the overidentified model. (p. 619) Therefore, a measure of goodnes of fit (Q) is calculated by taking the ratio between M and 32m as follows: Finally, Q is tested for significance using the W statistic which has an approximate x2 distribution with degrees of freedom equal to the number of path coefficients hypothesized to be equal to zero. W is calulated using the following equation: 153 fl='(N'Q)10g9Q Where: M = sample size d = number of path coefficients hypothesized to be zero Egg = the natural logarithm In the present study: 32m = 1 - (1- 321) (1- B22) (1— 8231(1- 1324) 1 - (.892)2 (.834)2 (.912)2 (.939)2 I50 to II 32 = 1 - (.795) (.696) (.832) (.882) g2 = 1 - .406 m 32m = .594 M = 1 — (1— 321) (1— R22) (1- 3231(1- 824) M = 1 - (.892)2 (.913)2 (.966)2 (.982)2 M = 1 - (.795) (.834) (.933) (.964) M = 1 - .596 M = .403 1- 32m Q = 1- M 1-.594 Q: = .680 1-.403 154 |€ =-(N-dllog§Q IS I! - (54 - 5) logQ .680 v_v = - (49) 10g; .680 IE = 18.90 Because the statistic V_V approximates a x2 distribution with degrees of freedom equal to the number of path coefficients hypothesized to be equal to zero: W = 712(5) = 18.90; p_< .01 155 APPENDIX N University Approval for Use of Human Subjects 3mm MICHIGAN STATE UNIVERSITY OFFIG OF VICE PRESIDENT FOR RESEARCH EAST LANSING 0 MICHIGAN 0 488244046 AND DEAN OF THE GRADUATE SCHOOL March 6, 1992 Richard R. Albrecht Office of Medical Education A-214 E. Fee Hall RE: THE INFLUENCE OF POSITIVE AND NEGATIVE COACHING STYLES ON THE COGNITIVE PRIMING 0F SUCCESS--AND FAILURE-RELATED SELF-SCHEMATA, SELF-EFFICACY AND MOTOR PERFORMANCE, 90—604 Dear Mr. Albrecht: UCRIHS' review of the above referenced project has now been completed. I am pleased to advise that the rights and welfare of the human subjects appear to be adequately protected and the Committee, therefore, has approved this project. You are reminded that UCRIHS approval is valid for one calendar year. If you plan to continue this project beyond one year, please make provisions for obtaining appropriate UCRIHS approval one month prior to March 2, 1993. There will be a maximum of four renewals possible. If you wish to continue a project beyond that time, it must again be submitted for complete review. Any changes in procedures involving human subjects must be reviewed by the UCRIHS prior to initiation of the change. UCRIHS must also be notified promptly of any problems (unexpected side effects, complaints, etc.) involving human subjects during the course of the work. Thank you for bringing this project to our attention. If we can be of any future help, please do not hesitate to let us know. Sincerely, Involving Human Subjects (UCRIHS) DEW/deo cc: Dr. Deborah Feltz MU”: a. All ,_.,.,. r. ‘ "1 I n r APPENDIX 0 Raw Data w TITLE "DISSERTATION” SUBTITLE "CLEANING FREQUENCIES" FILE HANDLE DATAl/NAME=“PHASE2 DATA.A1" DATA LIST FILE DATAl RECORDS = 9 /1 ID 1-3 RECl 4 AGE 6-7 RACE 8 CLASS 9 GPA 10 MAJOR 11-12 GOLF 13 GOLFYR 14 CLUBS 15 TEAM 16 COACH 17 PREDESl TO PREDESIO 19-28 PREIMPl TO PREIMPlO 30-39 /2 TENSEI 6 ANGRYl 7 WORNl 8 UNHAPl 9 LIVELYl 10 CONFUSl 11 PEEVEDI 12 /3 /5 /6 /7 /8 /9 SADl 13 ACTIVEl 14 EDGEl 15 GROUCHl l6 BLUEl 17 ENERGYl 18 HOPELESI 19 UNEASYI 20 RESTLESI 21 CONCENTl 22 FATIGl 23 ANNOYI 24 DISCOURl 25 RESENTl 26 NERVI 27 MISERI 28 CHEERl 29 BITTERl 30 EXHAUSTl 31 ANXI 32 HELPLSI 33 WEARYI 34 BEWILDl 35 FURYI 36 PEP1 37 WORTHLSI 38 FORGETl 39 VIGORl 40 UNCERTI 41 BUSHl 42 PRESEI TO PRESEZO 6-45 PRPERFl 6-7 PRPERFZ 9-10 PRPERF3 12-13 PRPERF4 15-16 PRPERFS 18-19 PRPERF6 21-22 PRPERF7 24-25 PRPERFB 27-28 PRPERF9 30-31 PRPERFlO 33-34 PRPERFll 36-37 PRPERFlZ 39-40 PRPERF13 42-43 PRPERF14 45-46 PRPERFlS 48-49 PRPERF16 51-52 PRPERF17 54-55 PRPERF18 57-58 PRPERF19 60-61 PRPERF20 63-64 ORDER 6-8 SCHEMA 10 COND 11 PSTDESI TO PSTDESlO 19-28 PSTIMPl TO PSTIMPlO 30-39 TENSE2 6 ANGRY2 7 WORN2 8 UNHAP2 9 LIVELYZ 10 CONFUSZ 11 PEEVEDZ 12 SAD2 13 ACTIVEZ 14 EDGEZ 15 GROUCHZ 16 BLUEZ 17 ENERGY2 18 HOPELESZ l9 UNEASY2 20 RESTLESZ 21 CONCENT2 22 FATIG2 23 ANNOYZ 24 DISCOUR2 25 RESENT2 26 NERV2 27 MISERZ 28 CHEER2 29 BITTER2 30 EXHAUSTZ 31 ANXZ 32 HELPLSZ 33 WEARYZ 34 BEWILDZ 35 FURYZ 36 PEP2 37 WORTHLSZ 38 FORGETZ 39 VIGORZ 40 UNCERT2 41 BUSHZ 42 PSTSEl TO PSTSE20 6-45 PTPERFl 6-7 PTPERFZ 9-10 PTPERF3 12-13 PTPERF4 15-16 PTPERFS 18-19 PTPERFG 21-22 PTPERF7 24-25 PTPERF8 27-28 PTPERF9 30-31 PTPERFlO 33-34 PTPERFll 36-37 PTPERF12 39-40 PTPERF13 42-43 PTPERF14 45-46 PTPERFlS 48-49 PTPERF16 51-52 PTPERF17 54-55 PTPERF18 57-58 PTPERF19 60-61 PTPERFZO 63-64 COLOR 6 TAPEl 7 TAPEZ 8 PUTTS 9 PURPOSE 10 FIRSTZO 11 NEXTZO 12 HOLEI 13 HOLE2 14 DOT 15 HELPHURT l6 POSNEG 17 VARIABLE LABELS GOLF GOLF PLAYED IN LIFETIME GOLFYR GOLF PLAYED IN LAST YR. CLUBS EVER OWN GOLF CLUBS? TEAM EVER PLAYED ON COMPET TEAM? COACH EXPERIENCE ON COMP.TEAM W COACH PREDESI PREDESZ PREDES3 PREDES4 PREDESS PREDES6 PREDES7 PREDESB PREDESQ PREDESlO PRETEST-CAN DO WHAT OTHERS CAN'T PREIMPl PREIMP2 PREIMP3 PREIMP4 PREIMPS PREIMP6 PREIMP7 PREIMP8 PREIMP9 PRETEST-EXCELLENT REFLEXES PRETEST-NOT AGILE AND GRACEFUL PRETEST-STRONG PHYSIQUE PRETEST-CAN'T RUN FAST PRETEST-NO PHYSICAL DEXTERITY PRETEST-POOR MUSCLE TONE PRETEST-LITTLE PRIDE IN ABILITY PRETEST-SPEED HAS HELPED ME PRETEST-STRONG GRIP PRETEST-EXCELLENT REFLEXES PRETEST-NOT AGILE AND GRACEFUL PRETEST-STRONG PHYSIQUE PRETEST-CAN'T RUN FAST PRETEST-NO PHYSICAL DEXTERITY PRETEST-POOR MUSCLE TONE PRETEST-LITTLE PRIDE IN ABILITY PRETEST-SPEED HAS HELPED ME PRETEST-STRONG GRIP 157 PREIMPlO PRETEST-CAN DO WHAT OTHERS CAN'T TENSEl PRETEST POMS-TENSE ANGRYI PRETEST POMS-ANGRY WORNl PRETEST POMS-WORN OUT UNHAPI PRESTEST POMS-UNHAPPY LIVELYl PRETEST POMS-LIVELY CONFUSl PRETEST POMS-CONFUSED PEEVEDl PRETEST POMS-PEEVED SADl PRETEST POMS-SAD ACTIVEl PRETEST POMS-ACTIVE EDGEl PRETEST POMS-ON EDGE GROUCHl PRETEST POMS-GROUCHY BLUEl PRETEST POMS-BLUE ‘ ENERGYl PRETEST POMS-ENERGETIC HOPELESl PRETEST POMS-HOPELESS UNEASYI PRETEST POMS-UNEASY RESTLES1 PRETEST POMS-RESTLESS CONCENTl PRETEST POMS-UNABLE TO CONCENTRATE FATIGl PRETEST POMS-FATIGUED ANNOYl PRETEST POMS-ANNOYED DISCOURI PRETEST POMS-DISCOURAGED RESENTl PRETEST POMS-RESENTFUL NERVl PRETEST POMS-NERVOUS MISERI PRETEST POMS-MISERABLE CHEERI PRETEST POMS-CHEERFUL BITTERl PRETEST POMS-BITTER EXHAUSTI PRETEST POMS-EXHAUSTED ANXl PRETEST POMS-ANXIOUS HELPLSI PRETEST POMS-HELPLESS WEARYl PRETEST POMS-WEARY BEWILDl PRETEST POMS-BEWILDERED FURYl PRETEST POMS-FURIOUS PEPl PRETEST POMS-PEP WORTHLSl PRETEST POMS-WORTHLESS FORGETl PRETEST POMS-FORGETFUL VIGORI PRETEST POMS-VIGOROUS UNCERTl PRETEST POMS-UNCERTAIN ABOUT THINGS BUSHl PRETEST POMS-BUSHED PRESEl PRE 1 OF 20 PRESEZ PRE 2 OF 20 PRESE3 PRE 3 OF 20 PRESE4 PRE 4 OF 20 PRESES PRE 5 OF 20 PRESE6 PRE 6 OF 20 PRESE7 PRE 7 OF 20 PRESEB PRE 8 OF 20 PRESE9 PRE 9 OF 20 PRESE10 PRE 10 OF 20 PRESEll PRE 11 OF 20 PRESE12 PRE 12 OF 20 PRESE13 PRE 13 OF 20 PRESE14 PRE 14 OF 20 PRESE15 PRE 15 OF 20 PRESE16 PRE 16 OF 20 PRESE17 PRE 17 OF 20 PRESE18 PRE 18 OF 20 PRESE19 PRE 19 OF 20 PRESEZO PRE 20 OF 20 PRPERFl PRE PERF TRIAL 1 PRPERF2 PRE PERF TRIAL 2 PRPERF3 PRE PERF TRIAL 3 PRPERF4 PRE PERF TRIAL 4 158 PRPERFS PRE PERF TRIAL 5 PRPERF6 PRE PERF TRIAL 6 PRPERF7 PRE PERF TRIAL 7 PRPERF8 PRE PERF TRIAL 8 PRPERF9 PRE PERF TRIAL 9 PRPERF10 PRE PERF TRIAL 10 PRPERFll PRE PERF TRIAL 11 PRPERF12 PRE PERF TRIAL 12 PRPERF13 PRE PERF TRIAL 13 PRPERF14 PRE PERF TRIAL l4 PRPERF15 PRE PERF TRIAL 15 PRPERF16 PRE PERF TRIAL l6 PRPERF17 PRE PERF TRIAL 17 PRPERF18 PRE PERF TRIAL 18 PRPERF19 PRE PERF TRIAL l9 PRPERF20 PRE PERF TRIAL 20 ORDER PRESENTATION ORDER SCHEMA SELF-SCHEMA TYPE COND TREATMENT CONDITION PSTDESl POSTTEST-EXCELLENT REFLEXES PSTDESZ POSTEST-NOT AGILE AND GRACEFUL PSTDES3 POSTEST-STRONG PHYSIQUE PSTDES4 POSTEST-CAN'T RUN FAST PSTDESS POSTEST-NO PHYSICAL DEXTERITY PSTDESS POSTEST-POOR MUSCLE TONE PSTDES7 POSTEST-LITTLE PRIDE IN ABILITY PSTDESB POSTEST-SPEED HAS HELPED ME PSTDES9 POSTEST-STRONG GRIP PSTDESlO POSTEST-CAN DO WHAT OTHERS CAN'T PSTIMPl POSTEST-EXCELLENT REFLEXES PSTIMP2 POSTEST-NOT AGILE AND GRACEFUL PSTIMP3 POSTEST-STRONG PHYSIQUE PSTIMP4 POSTEST-CAN'T RUN FAST PSTIMPS POSTEST-NO PHYSICAL DEXTERITY PSTIMP6 POSTEST-POOR MUSCLE TONE PSTIMP7 POSTEST-LITTLE PRIDE IN ABILITY PSTIMPB POSTEST-SPEED HAS HELPED ME PSTIMP9 POSTEST-STRONG GRIP PSTIMPlO POSTEST-CAN DO WHAT OTHERS CAN'T TENSE2 POSTEST POMS-TENSE ANGRYZ POSTEST POMS-ANGRY WORN2 POSTEST POMS-WORN OUT UNHAPZ POSSTEST POMS-UNHAPPY LIVELYZ POSTEST POMS-LIVELY CONFUS2 POSTEST POMS-CONFUSED PEEVEDZ PRETEST POMS-PEEVED SAD2 POSTEST POMS-SAD ACTIVEZ POSTEST POMS-ACTIVE EDGEZ POSTEST POMS-ON EDGE GROUCHZ POSTEST POMS-GROUCHY BLUE2 POSTEST POMS-BLUE ENERGYZ POSTEST POMS-ENERGETIC HOPELESZ POSTEST POMS-HOPELESS UNEASY2 POSTEST POMS-UNEASY RESTLESZ POSTEST POMS-RESTLESS CONCENT2 POSTEST POMS-UNABLE TO CONCENTRATE FATIGZ POSTEST POMS-FATIGUED ANNOYZ POSTEST POMS-ANNOYED DISCOUR2 POSTEST POMS-DISCOURAGED RESENT2 POSTEST POMS-RESENTFUL NERV2 POSTEST POMS-NERVOUS MISERZ POSTEST POMS-MISERABLE 159 CHEERZ POSTEST POMS-CHEERFUL BITTER2 POSTEST POMS-BITTER EXHAUSTZ POSTEST POMS-EXHAUSTED ANXZ POSTEST POMS-ANXIOUS HELPLSZ POSTEST POMS-HELPLESS WEARY2 POSTEST POMS-WEARY BEWILDZ POSTEST POMS-BEWILDERED FURYZ POSTEST POMS-FURIOUS PEP2 POSTEST POMS-PEP WORTHLSZ POSTEST POMS-WORTHLESS FORGETZ POSTEST POMS-FORGETFUL VIGOR2 POSTEST POMS-VIGOROUS UNCERT2 POSTEST POMS-UNCERTAIN ABOUT THINGS BUSH2 POSTEST POMS-BUSHED 0 PSTSEl POST 1 OF 2 PSTSEZ POST 2 OF 20 PSTSE3 POST 3 OF 20 PSTSE4 POST 4 OF 20 PSTSES POST 5 OF 20 PSTSE6 POST 6 OF 20 PSTSE7 POST 7 OF 20 PSTSE8 POST 8 OF 20 PSTSE9 POST 9 OF 20 PSTSE10 POST 10 OF 20 PSTSEll POST 11 OF 20 PSTSE12 POST 12 OF 20 PSTSE13 POST 13 OF 20 PSTSE14 POST 14 OF 20 PSTSE15 POST 15 OF 20 PSTSE16 POST 16 OF 20‘ PSTSE17 POST 17 OF 20 PSTSE18 POST 18 OF 20 PSTSE19 POST 19 OF 20 PSTSEZO POST 20 OF 20 PTPERFl POST PERF TRIAL PTPERFZ POST PERF TRIAL PTPERF3 POST PERF TRIAL PTPERF4 POST PERF TRIAL PTPERFS POST PERF TRIAL PTPERF6 POST PERF TRIAL PTPERF7 POST PERF TRIAL PTPERFB POST PERF TRIAL PTPERF9 POST PERF TRIAL PTPERFlo POST PERF TRIAL 10 PTPERFll POST PERF TRIAL 11 PTPERFlZ POST PERF TRIAL 12 PTPERF13 POST PERF TRIAL l3 PTPERF14 POST PERF TRIAL 14 PTPERF15 POST PERF TRIAL 15 PTPERF16 POST PERF TRIAL l6 PTPERF17 POST PERF TRIAL 17 PTPERF18 POST PERF TRIAL 18 PTPERF19 POST PERF TRIAL 19 PTPERF20 POST PERF TRIAL 20 COLOR COLOR OF BOX TAPEl WHAT WAS FIRST TAPE ABOUT? TAPE2 WHAT WAS SECOND TAPE ABOUT? PUTTS TOTAL NUMBER OF PUTTS TAKEN PURPOSE WHAT WAS PURPOSE OF STUDY? FIRST20 JUDGE YOUR PERF ON FIRST 20 PUTTS NEXTZO JUDGE YOUR PERF ON SECOND 20 PUTTS HOLEl HOW MANY PUTTS IN BOX ON FIRST 20? \DmflONU'I-bLQNH 160 HOLEZ HOW MANY PUTTS IN BOX ON SECOND 20? DOT WHAT WAS COLOR or BIODOT? HELPHURT DID WATCHING TAPE HELP.HURT OR NEITHER? POSNEG WAs THE COACH POSITIVE 0R NEGATIVE? RECODE PREIMPI To PREIMPlO (1:7) (2=6) (3=5) (4:4) RECODE PSTIMPl To PSTIMPIO (1:7) (2=6) (3=5) (4:4) RECODE PREDESl (1:7) (2=6) (3=6) (4=4) (5=3) (6=2) ( (6 = 2) (7 1) E (6 = 2) ) RECODE PREDES3 (1:7) (2=6) (3=6) (4=4) (5=3) (6=2) ( ( I (7;1 RECODE PREDESB (1:7) (2= 6) (3=6) (4= 4) (5= 3) (6= 2) RECODE PREDES9 (1:7) (2 =6) 3=6) (4:4) (5= 3) (6= 2) RECODE PREDESlO (1= 7) (2= -6) (3= 6) (4: 4) (5= —3) (6= -2) RECODE PSTDESI (1= 7) (2= —6) (3= 6) (4: —4) (5= -3) (6= 2) ( RECODE PSTDES3 (1:7) (2=6) (3=6) (4:4) (5=3) (6=2) ( RECODE PSTDESB (1:7) (2=6) (3=6) (4:4) (5=3) (6=2) (7 RECODE psroase (l=7) (2=6) (3:6) (4:4) (5=3) (6=2) (7= RECODE PSTDESlO (1:7) (2=6) (3=6) (4:4) (5=3) (6=2) (7= COMPUTE PRESCHl = PREDESl*PREIMP1 5= —3) 5= 3) 7= 1) 7=l) 7=1) 7=1) (7:1) 7=l) 7:1) :1) 1) l ) COMPUTE PRESCHZ = PREDESZ*PREIMP2 COMPUTE PRESCH3 = PREDES3*PREIMP3 COMPUTE PRESCH4 = PREDES4*PREIMP4 COMPUTE PRESCHS = PREDESS*PREIMP5 COMPUTE PRESCHG = PREDESG*PREIMP6 COMPUTE PRESCH7 = PREDES7*PREIMP7 COMPUTE PRESCH8 = PREDESB*PREIMP8 COMPUTE PRESCH9 = PREDES9*PREIMP9 COMPUTE PRESCHlO = PREDESlO*PREIMP10 COMPUTE PSTSCHl = PSTDESl*PSTIMP1 COMPUTE PSTSCHZ = PSTDESZ*PSTIMP2 COMPUTE PSTSCH3 = PSTDES3*PSTIMP3 COMPUTE PSTSCH4 = PSTDES4*PSTIMP4 COMPUTE PSTSCHS = PSTDESS*PSTIMP5 COMPUTE PSTSCH6 = PSTDESG*PSTIMP6 COMPUTE PSTSCH7 = PSTDES7*PSTIMP7 COMPUTE PSTSCH8 = PSTDES8*PSTIMP8 COMPUTE PSTSCH9 = PSTDES9*PSTIMP9 COMPUTE PSTSCHlO = PSTDESlO*PSTIMP10 COMPUTE PRESCHEM = PRESCH1+PRESCH2+PRESCH3+PRESCH4+PRESCH5+PRESCH6+ PRESCH7+PRESCH8+PRESCH9+PRESCH10 COMPUTE PSTSCHEM = PSTSCH1+PSTSCH2+PSTSCH3+PSTSCH4+PSTSCH5+PSTSCH6+ PSTSCH7+PSTSCH8+PSTSCH9+PSTSCH10 COMPUTE PREBLKl PRPERF1+PRPERF2+PRPERF3+PRPERF4+PRPERF5 COMPUTE PREBLKZ PRPERF6+PRPERF7+PRPERF8+PRPERF9+PRPERF10 COMPUTE PREBLK3 PRPERFl1+PRPERF12+PRPERF13+PRPERF14+PRPERF15 COMPUTE PREBLK4 PRPERFl6+PRPERF17+PRPERF18+PRPERF19+PRPERF20 COMPUTE TOTPERFl = PREBLK2+PREBLK3+PREBLK4 COMPUTE PSTBLKl = PTPERF1+PTPERF2+PTPERF3+PTPERF4+PTPERF5 COMPUTE PSTBLKZ = PTPERF6+PTPERF7+PTPERF8+PTPERF9+PTPERF10 COMPUTE PSTBLK3 = PTPERF11+PTPERF12+PTPERF13+PTPERF14+PTPERF15 COMPUTE PSTBLK4 = PTPERF16+PTPERF17+PTPERF18+PTPERF19+PTPERF20 COMPUTE TOTPERFZ = PSTBLK1+PSTBLK2+PSTBLK3 COMPUTE POMSTAl = TENSE1+EDGE1+UNEASY1+RESTLESl+NERVl+ANX1 COMPUTE POMSDDl = UNHAP1+SADI+BLUE1+HOPELESl+DISCOUR1+ MISERl+HELPLSl+WORTHLSl COMPUTE POMSAHl = ANGRYl+PEEVED1+GROUCH1+ANNOY1+RESENT1+BITTER1+FURY1 COMPUTE POMSVAl LIVELY1+ACTIVE1+ENERGY1+CHEER1+PEP1+VIGOR1 COMPUTE POMSFIl WORNl+FATIG1+EXHAUST1+WEARY1+BUSH1 COMPUTE POMSCBl CONFUS1+CONCENT1+BEWILD1+FORGET1+UNCERT1 COMPUTE POMSTAZ TENSE2+EDGE2+UNEASY2+RESTLES2+NERV2+ANX2 COMPUTE POMSDDZ UNHAP2+SADZ+BLUE2+HOPELESZ+DISCOUR2+ MISER2+HELPLSZ+WORTHLSZ COMPUTE POMSAHZ = ANGRY2+PEEVED2+GROUCH2+ANNOY2+RESENT2+BITTER2+FURY2 COMPUTE POMSVAZ = LIVELY2+ACTIVE2+ENERGY2+CHEER2+PEP2+VIGOR2 II II II II 161 COMPUTE POMSFIZ WORN2+FATIG2+EXHAUST2+WEARY2+BUSH2 COMPUTE POMSCBZ CONFUSZ+CONCENT2+BEWILD2+FORGET2+UNCERT2 COMPUTE POMSTMDl = POMSTA1+POMSDDI+POMSAH1+POMSFI1+POMSCB1-POMSVA1 COMPUTE POMSTMD2 = POMSTA2+POMSDD2+POMSAHZ+POMSFI2+POMSCBZ-POMSVA2 COMPUTE SE1 = PRESEI+PRESE2+PRESE3+PRESE4+PRESE5+ PRESE6+PRESE7+PRESE8+PRESE9+PRESE10+ PRESE11+PRESE12+PRESE13+PRESE14+PRESE15+ PRESEl6+PRESE17+PRESE18+PRESE19+PRESE20 PSTSE1+PSTSE2+PSTSE3+PSTSE4+PSTSE5+ PSTSE6+PSTSE7+PSTSE8+PSTSE9+PSTSE10+ PSTSE11+PSTSE12+PSTSE13+PSTSE14+PSTSE15+ PSTSEI6+PSTSE17+PSTSE18+PSTSE19+PSTSE20 COMPUTE SCHEMDIF = PRESCHEM - PSTSCHEM COMPUTE SEDIF = SE1-SE2 COMPUTE POMSDIF = POMSTMDl-POMSTMDZ COMPUTE PERFDIF = TOTPERFl-TOTPERFZ RECODE ORDER (123=1) (132=2) (213=3) ( IF (COND E0 1) OR (COND E0 3) DUMMY IF (COND E0 2) OR (COND E0 4) DUMMY FINISH COMPUTE SE2 231=4) (312=5) (321=6) 0 1 162 18612 51122 4453322656 6314213464 2121311111112111121111121111111211111 IUIUI ““““ 403030707070701010101 31 50 14 18 35 18 11 13 27 13 28 16 50 18 312 22 4363322566 6313212663 1121211121112111121111121211111211211 1010090908070706060504040303020202010101 47 50 50 50 50 50 29 18 24 50 50 22 21 50 111113300121 18612 42111 5644445446 1114311123 2111111132113123111212131121111111211 0909090909090806060504030303030302020101 26 50 25 13 50 35 24 44 43 50 20 41 10 36 321 21 5544346445 1122212114 1112211131113111111211131121111211211 1010101010100908070604030202010101010101 09 11 30 16 20 25 10 08 28 50 16 44 42 42 111113200135 19612 53125 3333566423 2213111321 2111411141113111131111141121111211111 0909090808080707070606050504030302020101 14 50 23 15 50 36 17 ll 38 50 23 50 16 50 123 13 3334666422 3223221322 1111211121112112131111131111111211111 1010090909080808070706050404030302010101 50 50 34 32 38 24 50 21 50 50 50 24 16 08 111112110135 20633 52124 2136566343 3623213362 2131311132112112211111141231111113122 0909090807060505040403030303020201010101 50 25 21 50 50 27 06 50 18 10 09 17 20 50 312 22 2136563552 3623423463 2121221131112122111111131121121112222 0605050505040403030202020202020101010101 17 30 07 41 10 28 07 08 15 21 50 18 14 18 111115389131 18611 21125 2446557464 2143212452 2124311132114122122212131131111311132 0808070706060505040404040302020202010101 50 50 50 50 44 50 50 26 29 50 35 50 36 22 231 13 2456566463 2132212452 2124311132114122122212131131111311132 0606060504040404030303030202020201010101 39 22 39 17 07 10 10 39 27 28 13 48 32 12 111111300125 19622 53225 1622677141 1131321142 711 73111 331 7712311 77771 37171171711231 1010101010101010100908080707070605040404 42 08 10 44 12 50 50 50 18 30 19 39 50 35 213 11 1626667131 1131221131 2212321231123123112221142131121211221 1010101010101010101009080707060606050403 20 12 50 39 26 07 28 45 07 40 18 46 09 50 111113400135 18612 52125 3736777222 1111111221 2121321121112122121112121221111211122 0908080707050403030302020202010101010101 03 50 50 20 27 26 18 32 34 25 09 39 37 21 312 11 2727777221 1111111221 2131221123112131121113131231111111122 163 14 35 50 50 21 43 02 38 39 26 16 08 36 24 16 40 11 50 50 12 40 11 50 50 15 l009090908080707060504040303030202020202 16 37 15 50 50 13 43 30 23 25 20 12 50 13 111113200145 18613 64124 3651456656 3435223663 1342311242123112231111131221111211111 0707070706050403030303030303020101010101 30 10 44 50 50 25 12 29 50 50 24 50 29 04 213 22 3651555755 3435433653 1121311131113113121111131771111711111 1010090807050503030302020202010101010101 49 30 50 22 50 15 25 32 21 17 18 18 36 19 111112300141 19623 62122 5325234324 2234424334 3121111112211123151113111421411112135 1010101009090807050403030302020202010101 09 37 38 43 14 29 15 27 50 50 50 50 50 50 321 13 5325334423 3234424333 2151121113211123252212111511421113134 0909090908080808070605050505050504040404 36 50 17 16 50 08 41 09 25 18 11 50 26 36 111111300133 17613 62123 5261221444 2431425141 2112223221112111123412214252222324352 1008060404040302020201010101010101010101 34 37 31 11 50 32 17 50 31 50 11 28 45 38 123 21 5441423735 2443126142 3242123132123112332222142221211225352 1010101009090908070706060404030302020101 50 05 11 22 03 10 16 50 11 50 21 50 18 50 111111100115 18612 52111 3455343355 3343334443 2121211122112122221112131111211111121 0303020202020202020101010101010101010101 50 50 50 21 50 27 20 50 17 06 50 27 23 27 213 23 3555443355 3343354343 1111211122111111121111111111111111111 O404040404040303030303030303030303010101 22 50 40 08 50 47 07 50 36 37 13 26 33 18 111113300134 18623 21111 2123366551 1115415251 1131211121112121221112141221111113121 0505050404030303020202020202020101010101 42 31 50 39 23 50 50 22 20 31 32 19 19 50 231 22 1723363663 1116315262 1121111121111112221111141211111112121 0808080505050503030302010101010101010101 29 25 50 39 12 37 23 31 45 50 13 50 35 35 111113300131 18612 42122 4344433765 5355425553 1131211112112111221111121321111211122 1009080706050403020202010101010101010101 21 50 49 41 20 50 44 32 50 14 07 21 50 18 123 33 5345435775 6345435554 1131211112212111541211121221111111122 0707060605030301010101010101010101010101 35 32 50 50 50 50 50 50 50 12 40 31 50 42 111111200133 18611 32123 2726666323 2223213223 2122421142115123231112141221221311222 1010101009090909090909080706050404020202 50 50 28 27 50 33 18 37 50 50 50 29 30 40 213 11 2627666323 2222213123 164 11 11 23 20 10 50 50 17 18 50 40 36 14 50 15 4O 13 39 14 06 16 50 28 50 28 13 32 50 14 50 31 35 20 0996 0997 0998 0999 1011 1012 1013 1014 1015 1016 1017 1018 1019 1021 1022 1023 1023 1025 1026 1027 1024 1029 1041 1042 1043 1044 1045 1046 1047 1048 1049 1171 1172 1173 1174 1175 1176 1177 1178 1179 1201 1202 1203 1204 1205 1206 1207 1208 1209 1211 1212 1213 1214 1215 1216 1217 1218 1219 1221 1222 1223 1224 2222412142115122222211141221211311222 1010101010101009090808070706050403030202 14 50 12 25 27 50 31 38 40 35 08 36 16 44 111211199135 18612 65123 4535544534 3334213434 1111311131113111111111141111111311211 1010101010100909090909090808080806050404 50 07 50 23 50 20 18 44 50 50 50 35 24 27 213 32 4544455534 3334323434 1111311131113111111111131111111211211 1010101010080808080706060606050504030202 11 29 26 10 50 43 07 50 21 50 50 14 50 25 111111300131 21642 52211 2636676323 2223213332 1111211121113111111112131121111211111 0909080604040303020101010101010101010101 06 44 50 13 35 13 33 29 34 50 12 10 33 50 123 12 2736776223 1233213333 1111211121112111111111131121111211111 1010101008070606050504040303010101010101 35 27 09 50 50 19 13 44 16 26 06 50 30 24 111114400131 19624 63124 3454544555 3223312343 1131211121112111141111131311311212113 0909090908070707060605050404030302020202 12 20 19 22 50 50 41 25 33 50 37 46 16 45 321 21 3344444555 2222211232 1111211121112111131111121311211211113 1010101009090808070605050504030303020202 11 20 16 12 45 36 11 22 30 50 26 24 06 42 111113301135 18611 66225 2521656212 2211211112 1111311131113112211111131121111211111 1010100808070706060505050404040303030202 50 12 25 35 43 37 45 33 12 33 16 25 12 15 321 31 2521667212 1111211212 ll11311131112112111111121121111211111 1010100909090908080707070605050504040302 19 15 50 44 34 50 50 14 30 33 50 13 33 18 111113410124 18611 53123 3332666335 6236513671 2131311221112111132112121231211111142 1009090707050503030302010101010101010101 50 50 50 50 50 50 27 50 50 50 20 10 48 22 312 31 3326565555 5226613571 1121211221111111131211121221211111122 1010101010070706050503030303030303030302 15 44 32 46 13 26 16 39 12 50 23 22 50 16 111111100125 18612 41122 6275235577 5564654666 2131211121112111231112121221111112111 0707050505040403030303030303030302020101 50 11 15 12 12 48 28 16 03 50 34 50 08 37 312 33 6274535566 6664545666 1121211121112111211111121111111111111 0707060505040404040403030303030303020101 42 08 40 50 16 50 18 50 50 10 13 48 14 17 111113210132 18612 41124 2623627424 1225211325 1111111121113111111111121111111111111 0908080807070606060606050505040403010101 50 31 29 50 50 50 24 11 35 21 13 50 34 34 165 28 50 50 50 20 27 26 18 18 11 38 23 50 50 37 17 30 07 11 25 21 50 43 37 50 08 30 34 42 15 50 50 50 12 10 15 50 32 50 50 22 41 35 50 11 41 21 27 21 50 43 50 45 39 24 14 50 12 14 20 15 24 24 50 12 21 50 50 40 33 50 31 13 50 24 36 36 43 05 50 50 36 50 48 1225 1226 1227 1228 1229 1231 1232 1233 1234 1235 1236 1237 1238 1239 2051 2052 2053 2054 2055 2056 2057 2058 2059 2121 2122 2123 2124 2125 2126 2127 2128 2129‘ 2321 2322 2323 2324 2325 2326 2327 2328 2329 2491 2492 2493 2494 2495 2496 2497 2498 2499 2721 2722 2723 2724 2725 2726 2727 2728 2729 2941 2942 2943 132 12 2623667325 1225222336 1121211121112111111111131211111111111 0908080706060505040403020201010101010101 25 20 25 19 50 50 37 50 05 07 32 48 23 22 111111301121 18612 53122 2656455672 1115111641 1141411111113111141111131511111211114 0909080806060605050303030303010101010101 14 38 50 50 50 18 49 37 45 25 36 38 40 50 132 11 2732456552 1116114441 1141211111112111151111131111111211114 1010101010101009090808080808070706060503 50 12 23 50 50 27 50 40 50 50 50 50 50 23 111114300135 19623 52123 3551545722 3433121341 2142221223112122141112131421111211223 0808080808060404040404030303010101010101 50 50 44 32 45 22 08 11 32 41 22 16 13 22 312 32 4541656731 3537222531 2141311132113123141212131421221311222 1008080606060604020201010101010101010101 32 38 16 12 50 32 50 24 28 07 36 28 35 44 111111100131 19622 42111 3534264723 2213316443 2141211122122111231212121311221112223 1010100909080707060605040403030301010101 50 50 50 18 50 44 18 50 34 22 20 16 13 23 321 23 2633464722 2214316432 2142132112121322241311121313221122142 1010080707070606060604040403030202020101 15 38 13 33 50 22 36 44 50 05 13 24 30 20 111114400133 20633 52122 2642464663 6355423364 2142111211122112231122231211111111112 0505050505050504040404030303020202010101 50 38 42 12 50 50 50 24 38 12 10 11 50 50 231 21 2543465644 4245423354 2132111221122112131121121311111111113 0808080706050403030303030202020101010101 23 22 11 12 50 11 22 42 32 15 10 45 50 34 111112400134 18611 32123 4452254654 3323213332 1142111111211111441111111411111111114 1010090807060605040303030303030202010101 50 50 50 44 43 18 07 21 50 47 24.17 08 04 132 23 4462235656 4322223432 ll31111111112111331211121311111111113 1010101010090908080707070606050302020101 22 35 18 50 23 27 18 46 35 08 06 15 50 33 111112300133 19623 42125 6647757735 6667767736 2132321132113122131112231321221311323 0706060504030201010101010101010101010101 50 50 30 50 02 06 17 28 50 50 50 34 12 50 321 31 5647647735 6647756736 1131311131113121121211131321211212223 0807050404040404030201010101010101010101 06 18 09 18 43 07 18 50 48 13 12 25 50 50 ‘121111200135 18612 63125 4523667434 4637111375 1121311131123122251111221421311111114 1009090805050404040403030303030202010101 166 27 31 50 08 50 41 ll 16 50 44 12 50 45 12 20 12 35 44 32 18 11 10 35 44 50 50 17 40 16 50 32 22 29 32 19 16 23 38 50 21 19 17 26 50 07 42 07 50 33 28 50 50 36 50 23 37 24 21 50 31 14 21 42 36 22 34 50 39 20 12 28 22 50 144 41 50 36 31 36 50 20 16 47 12 46 21 23 44 45 06 09 29 312 32 5223557545 5545411666 1231111112121112242211211421211121123 ‘ ‘ IUIUIU[0305040404030303030301 25 29 11 12 32 15 16 29 43 35 03 27 32 24 111112401131 18612 42124 5335464534 5345525555 1121211121111112121111121121111112111 1010100908080706060504040403020202020202 32 14 08 26 11 17 38 44 23 50 11 43 03 26 321 32 4325566535 5434434444 1121111111111111121111111111111112111 1010100807060504040403030303020201010101 50 44 13 50 21 25 18 22 07 41 50 15 50 28 111112100131 18613 63125 1417777111 1311111111 2121431141114221113112241241141411342 1010101010101010101010100606060303030303 34 33 30 15 30 27 22 31 19 44 15 04 26 31 213 12 3437577113 1111111111 3222324143214111212212232141131311221 1010101010101005050403010101010101010101 35 32 50 47 22 50 28 44 18 11 30 41 44 42 111113222131 18613 52123 4363643373 3343313362 213l211111111111121112131211111111131 0909090807060503030202020101010101010101 35 33 25 13 45 33 50 36 21 50 20 11 42 41 132 21 3463654363 3333313252 1121111111111112121211131221111111121 1010101009080705040403030303020201010101 50 05 04 33 27 13 25 46 22 18 28 50 46 38 5111113300135 18612 11122 6241211746 6343344243 2121411142114114111111131121111411211 0707070705040402020202020202020202020202 50 50 50 50 50 24 06 21 50 38 18 26 45 22 132 23 2141212747 3343224141 2111411142113113111112141121111411211 1010080707060504040403030303030202020202 50 28 33 17 50 50 14 22 50 50 38 30 50 42 [121112298134 18613 43111 4665554565 4354334455 1121211222111111231111111311211111122 0909090808070706060605040403020201010101 45 25 50 13 32 50 18 25 22 17 34 25 25 07 132 31 4553433563 4354346354 1121212212211111122111111211211111122 1010101010100908070605050404030302020101 45 14 18 21 36 13 22 50 11 23 08 01 11 19 '121113400124 18611 43125 1536667143 1211311147 1131211131113114121111121211211111111 0505050505050505050403030303030303020201 38 08 09 16 50 27 ll 36 26 30 32 32 29 11 312 11 1636567143 1211311147 1121211111111112121111121111111111111 0605050505050505050504030303020202020201 50 50 41 34 24 50 28 08 26 46 40 16 16 23 111111410131 17612 65125 2727777222 1121111122 2121311132113122111112131121111211211 167 42 35 32 23 42 28 35 33 22 47 50 14 '42 21 41 14 ll 31 39 27 33 50 20 11 07 22 22 24 44 15 25 39 3463 3464 3465 3466 3467 3468 3469 3481 3482 3483 3484 3485 3486 3487 3488 3489 3541 3542 3543 3544 3545 3546 3547 3548 3549 3571 3572 3573 3574 3575 3576 3577 3578 3579 3631 3632 1010101010090808070705050504030302020201 44 17 35 50 41 22 08 24 37 19 13 07 33 38 123 12 2127777221 1121111121 2111311132113121111112131121111211221 1010101009080606060504030303020202020101 50 16 31 50 33 19 29 24 42 50 17 23 05 14 111111100141 18612 21113 4532563666 3245435575 1111111121111111121111121131111111111 0909090505030303030303030303020202020101 50 21 44 23 50 14 36 22 18 31 12 44 50 36 132 33 3543564554 3245434565 1121111121112111121111131111211111111 1010100909080806060504040404030303030201 12 36 44 25 36 41 21 07 25 33 15 42 50 33 111114310135 18612 64125 4662424775 5556735363 2131211121113111131112141121211211211 0909090909090808080707070606050505040403 50 50 44 50 31 20 15 11 50 49 35 23 26 13 132 31 6572563774 7667635373 1121311121112122121112141121211211311 1010101010101008080606060505040303020201 25 33 18 01 40 25 44 18 11 08 09 25 17 35 111113200145 19624 31125 4423566243 3232211242 2123121211221222231213211321121111131 1007050302010101010101010101010101010101 50 44 22 50 24 36 38 50 50 50 30 11 50 50 213 22 4536656542 2232221342 1132121111221211131111111211111111121 0503030301010101010101010101010101010101 26 11 17 50 50 34 32 10 50 50 50 19 46 50 111112200131 19622 52122 4556564676 6577657675 3141221122111121141112121411411111114 0606060505050404040303030302020202010101 33 50 33 50 44 18 35 25 21 46 13 03 25 17 213 33 6566554776 6566667676 2141111112111122141211111411311111123 0707070504040303030202020202020202010101 50 35 18 35 15 46 34 08 22 45 14 33 13 22 111112300133 18613 32112 3546635534 3345335422 2121411131113113111112141121111311221 0605050504040403030303020202010101010101 28 14 50 30 28 50 07 13 12 50 32 18 50 50 213 32 3535434523 2345434422 2111311141113112211112141111111411121 1010090808060604040303030303020201010101 10 46 50 03 50 24 17 42 41 50 50 25 27 26 '111113200131 18611 62125 2727777221 1112111323 2131311131112111131112131211111211212 1010101009080605050404030303020202010101 50 50 50 50 35 44 22 18 37 29 22 07 44 36 231 13 2727777323 1112111323 1131311121112111131112131211111211212 1010100909070605050404030303020101010101 50 44 19 43 21 18 15 39 50 21 33 14 36 35 ~111113100135 20224 4532646313 3334111222 168 26 11 41 50 07 39 07 50 37 36 50 50 41 18 31 36 26 14 40 41 35 49 ll 22 13 50 50 07 32 38 13 21 11 21 13 26 32 39 50 10 28 22 23 16 44 33 32 15 50 33 10 14 50 50 14 18 23 32 19 08 17 17 50 41 42 15 50 19 36 25 41 35 23 28 23 25 14 26 36 40 35 50 21 40 5922 5923 5924 5925 5926 5927 5928 5929 7011 7012 7013 7014 7015 7016 7017 7018 7019 7021 7022 7023 7024 7025 7026 7027 7028 7029 7031 7032 7033 7034 7035 7036 7037 7038 7039‘ 7051 7052 7053 7054 7055 7056 7057 7058 7059‘ 7061 7062 7063 7064 7065 7066 7067 7068 7069 7081 7082 7083 7084 7085 7086 7087 7088 7089 1123221221142113242111121431231114332 0909080705040404040303030303030303030303 50 50 45 28 50 35 44 33 26 45 19 50 45 47 321 21 4456747312 3333121222 1122211221132112241111121421111113244 0909080705040404040303030303030303030303 44 45 26 28 33 50 29 30 18 09 28 40 42 17 111113400131 20632 66123 2656555654 3333323244 4153211433143112341111231521311112214 1010101010101010101010101009090908080807 21 37 13 31 45 14 16 25 36 06 42 21 06 11 123 31 2355553454 3333434344 2153211422142112251111121521411112135 1010101010101010101009090909080808070707 11 10 20 05 00 19 16 15 22 08 27 17 29 10 111112301134 21632 52125 4555533364 3232223354 2122312131213112221111131121211312321 0504030201010101010101010101010101010101 07 17 28 50 38 04 35 32 20 21 37 42 50 16 123 11 5545545464 3222223454 1121311131113112211111131121211312321 0909090806050503020202010101010101010101 36 36 17 16 23 21 10 45 29 12 36 50 26 04 111114300131 19612 42123 3335455534 1224312232 2111411132113121121112131131211212211 0806050505040302020202020202020202020101 12 07 22 32 09 05 04 22 17 50 15 50 31 50 123 12 3235553434 1224312444 3123311143214123221112131131211212231 0807060604030303030302020202010101010101 08 50 20 50 50 40 19 50 50 25 50 10 10 19 111111400041 19612 52125 2342335565 3333322434 3121311131112132131112121121111112122 0909070606050504040404040403030303030302 39 50 36 38 30 23 39 31 29 09 13 30 31 38 213 31 3242325555 3323322424 2111111112121121121212111121111111111 0909080808060604040404030303030303020202 42 50 35 03 21 10 15 08 50 23 50 09 21 16 111111300125 19613 32124 3655443462 3243434243 2433122322221211233311221112211122241 0909090808080706060505050505030303020101 41 50 01 50 50 16 13 03 17 31 10 27 13 50 132 33 2645433343 3243434233 2434223332222212234312221112211122242 0808070606060505050504040404040303030201 32 12 21 12 35 35 13 15 29 42 17 09 19 25 111112200134 20622 65225 2524666333 2224322334 1121311131113111121111131211111112111 0808070707070705050505050404040303020101 27 20 22 09 19 31 32 18 18 15 14 08 35 25 123 13 2624662334 2324222434 1121311121111111121111121211111112111 0909080808070706050505040404030302020101 08 17 38 16 37 10 25 09 09 25 37 22 12 17 122123200133 169 35 26 29 42 31 11 27 23 18 41 10 28 20 30 25 17 10 10 28 39 50 18 19 25 34 16 17 22 23 47 13 11 41 16 50 30 02 12 07 37 12 35 18 28 11 07 06 11 50 24 13 35 36 18 10 46 44 32 14 16 31 08 36 22 29 12 20 13 12 35 46 37 14 10 11 40 27 08 04 13 14 13 20 40 19614 51224 3535566334 1123311324 1141212121113111341112141321111212223 1010090707070707070706050505030302020101 34 31 24 20 41 16 26 27 30 32 12 17 02 42 123 12 2335456334 1122211343 1141213121112112341211121421111213213 1010100808050403030302020201010101010101 31 35 50 47 25 31 50 33 50 28 29 37 50 38 111111301121 19611 41125 3426567142 3311211142 3123432353124334322313141231221421342 1010090908070605050404040403030202020101 43 40 50 14 40 07 36 23 44 45 29 18 50 05 231 13 2326366142 2311211131 3123431252224323421212241233221521332 1010101010101010101010090907070706060504 13 04 15 20 17 18 28 40 02 19 13 08 11 15 111111400133 19112 31124 3634456523 4235313444 2121411131113111231212131321311211122 1010090807070606050505040404040303020101 50 22 50 11 13 50 11 44 10 08 10 11 46 02 213 33 4643445633 4234323544 2121311131114111231212131221111211122 1010101010090807060606060505040302020101 06 10 02 12 47 02 28 10 11 05 50 02 15 23 111112401125 18611 52124 3545626423 2112211232 2121312132213122222111141221211211112 1010090908080707060605050404040404040302 50 21 50 50 29 50 18 14 40 13 15 11 18 10 231 22 2543514523 2112211232 2111212121112112111111141111211111111 1010101010100909080807070606050504040303 05 29 50 20 14 13 16 20 21 22 50 50 18 11 111113200141 18613 66225 3626267222 3322121133 ll21211141113111321111141121111111221 0909090909080808070707070606060606050303 37 37 37 12 10 20 13 17 13 22 20 13 34 03 123 12 2636767721 3344121133 2111211131117111111211171171111111771 1010090909080707060606050505040303030302 21 30 19 22 ll 05 29 20 02 10 03 29 08 06 211113201151 33642 42121 1111777117 1711111111 2131311131113122121112131321221311321 1010101010090807050404040303030303030303 35 50 34 04 09 16 18 17 50 06 23 26 15 09 312 11 1111777117 1711111111 1121311131113177111117131771111311371 l010101010090909080605050404030303030303 17 31 38 40 21 03 13 28 45 16 03 27 16 16 111113300133 19611 53122 2555466464 2223323254 3121311132113122221112131221111211222 0707060606050504030303030303020202020202 43 40 13 48 50 02 09 14 50 23 22 09 17 11 312 31 2646566463 2223422255 2121211131113111221112131111111211212 1009090909080807060605050404030202020201 22 19 15 21 06 49 32 37 12 21 15 14 31 16 170 18 26 13 31 20 13 23 28 25 37 20 22 37 35 13 07 50 31 04 08 50 15 11 12 11 21 14 49 04 12 26 10 15 17 21 50 23 10 08 7269 7281 7282 7283 7284 7285 7286 7287 7288 7289 7291 7292 7293 7294 7295 7296 7297 7298 7299' 7371 7372 7373 7374 7375 7376 7377 7378 7379 7441 7442 7443 7444 7445 7446 7447 7448 7449. 7491 7492 7493 7494 7495 7496 7497 7498 7499 7641 7642 7643 7644 7645 7646 7647 7648 7649 7651 . 7652 7653 7654 7655 7656 7657 111113300134 20632 52125 2644547342 1121321233 2141211121112212241112121321311111124 1010090807070606050505040403030302020101 20 50 06 34 20 15 29 20 28 12 23 19 16 22 321 11 3544546342 2131321232 1131211121112211231211121321311111213 1010100908080707060505040403020202010101 36 15 14 45 08 32 41 48 14 14 12 08 23 09 111112300124 19612 41125 2727666111 2222222221 2121311131113111121112141221211211122 1010101010090808080707070707070706060606 30 50 29 29 11 25 14 18 11 50 20 26 33 02 213 13 2727766121 2222222221 1121311131113111121111141211111211121 1010101010101010101009090909080707060606 47 35 14 10 50 45 05 14 30 50 01 03 07 09 111114414134 18611 31122 3536655434 3323414343 2121311131113111121211141211111311211 0808070706060605050505040404040303030202 17 07 50 27 25 29 39 33 39 40 50 16 19 25 213 32 3636665633 3323314333 2111311131113111111211141111111311211 1010101010100909090808080808080805040302 28 03 50 07 50 21 13 24 29 50 45 06 02 32 111115312141 19612 53122 3353543665 2333222432 1121311121113112131111131221211112212 1010101010101009070705050505040302020201 18 36 08 18 27 18 21 21 24 13 15 28 11 23 213 21 3344434554 2333222443 1121311121112111121111131211211111112 1010101010101010101010090909080807040302 09 06 07 20 19 30 11 30 12 08 02 11 14 12 111113200135 19613 63124 2326677223 2532321222 2122312142114122111112131231211311221 1010101009090808080808070706060606050404 50 50 28 11 50 23 08 22 03 22 19 03 00 31 312 32 2326667233 2522321333 2211312132113112112212131131111311211 1010101009090808070606050404030302020101 03 03 24 03 43 23 34 16 10 20 50 20 12 20 111112324123 19621 21125 2624364225 3512311311 2122332424123213222311121234312341151 0908080806050404040403030303020202010101 50 50 44 27 13 00 27 37 11 50 06 42 43 02 231 33 3225165325 3512313423 3212122234324434313322321134311232341 0806050404030302010101010101010101010101 08 21 35 13 50 15 02 15 13 11 50 11 27 32 111111210131 18211 21122 3743445326 1121123116 3121111122111122151112121521211111114 0606060505040404040303030303030303030201 50 07 50 50 21 50 27 17 50 50 13 14 26 43 231 23 4644445447 1121124117 3131111112111112151112111421211111114 0202020202020202020202020202010101010101 171 14 22 24 12 19 46 16 15 28 29 29 12 50 28 18 25 19 02 06 20 12 21 15 35 34 37 26 16 21 23 07 17 10 10 08 13 34 20 50 45 24 20 25 15 22 09 07 12 01 50 50 11 50 50 18 13 34 09 22 25 06 23 27 23 07 06 03'07 10 02 17 01 50 42 31 43‘ 50 23 7865 7866 7867 7868 7869 7901 7902 7903 7904 7905 7906 25 30 32 50 45 05 37 24 42 40 25 50 26 04 111113501134 19613 43123 4536555333 5323423344 2122312132113123312212141121211313321 1010101010101010101009090908080808070706 20 19 29 32 37 13 50 35 10 50 09 15 21 50 213 23 4535555333 4423523344 2122311132113123111112131121211311311 1010101010101010101010101010090908080706 24 05 40 50 32 24 25 39 25 29 03 08 50 22 .111213302134 21643 42122 2626464242 1112212131 2111421141115112111112141111111411421 1010101010101009090907070606050404030303 03 44 28 25 03 17 47 14 19 23 16 13 15 25 312 12 2636464242 1112212121 1111321141114112111112151111111411421 1010101010100907070605050504040303030202 13 50 29 13 06 03 32 09 23 11 20 03 09 21 111113230142 19622 41122 2645223664 2113213322 3243122312221233442222211331411122124 0909090808080808080807070706060605040303 26 50 05 13 50 50 09 50 13 07 27 05 13 50 321 22 2653213564 2112314322 12132122313331222242212211431411121134 1010101010080707060505040404030302020101 31 35 38 26 17 45 50 07 19 11 20 23 20 27 111112310134 19611 32125 2627661121 1111112152 2111412142114111112121141111111411311 l010090807060505040404040403030303030202 18 50 33 34 31 50 31 09 30 21 04 23 25 28 213 13 2627666131 1111212142 2111312142113112112211131111111311311 1008080706060505040403030302020201010101 40 08 50 50 36 31 20 21 17 21 50 35 25 25 111112300134 19622 63124 4534555623 3335312543 1121311141114113121112141111111311311 1009090808080807070707070707060606050505 50 25 20 35 44 26 50 20 50 43 11 05 50 14 132 22 3535552523 2225312534 1111311131114111121112141111111311311 0808080707070606060505050504040404040403 24 50 50 15 16 21 08 14 50 31 35 48 22 12 111112423131 20232 11123 3335465223 4443213265 1132211122112111221112121221211211222 0909070606060605040302020202020202020202 50 50 50 30 50 08 50 45 14 18 19 50 42 09 231 32 6434454334 3322424344 2122111122112121221212121221222211222 0505040302020202020202020101010101010101 15 50 50 28 25 25 40 15 20 35 18 45 14 10 11111111100033 20622 32122 4423536624 3344223343 2131411143124122121112151331111412434 0808070707060606050504040303030302020101 50 42 50 42 50 09 02 50 50 50 24 16 07 02 132 23 4424335525 4344223343 2121411142114112121111151221111412422 172 39 29 50 02 22 37 21 50 50 37 35 24 50 50 34 25 21 07 01 27 29 11 12 ll 50 40 09 46 11 28 15 25 02 34 15 48 42 50 08 20 50 11 46 31 26 11 19 02 29 50 20 38 50 10 42 50 50 22 47 17 12 34 27 06 16 19 07 45 16 50 50 50 08' 25 22 37 22 31 48 18 32 12 46 19 7907 7908 7909 7961 7962 7963 7964 7965 7966 7967 7968 7969 7981 7982 7983 7984 7985 7986 7987 7988 7989 7991 7992 7993 7994 7995 7996 7997 7998 7999 8011 8012 8013 8014 8015 8016 8017 8018 8019 8041 8042 8043 8044 8045 8046 8047 8048 8049 8061 8062 8063 8064 8065 8066 8067 8068 8069 8081 8082 8083 8084 8085 1009090807060504040303030302020201010101 18 25 14 14 33 12 15 14 27 08 11 50 34 03 111112311134 18614 66225 3321675712 4536414423 3121221131312111111111121131111211131 0909090908080808080707070707060505030202 50 50 50 20 11 13 41 50 50 27 18 41 18 14 321 33 3423575613 3514313523 2111211121212111111111121131111211111 1010090909080807070707060605050404030303 12 09 21 11 16 18 50 36 10 50 05 09 38 23 111113300132 19612 62125 2734367432 2224211221 4142211122111121231212131221211111242 1010101010090909090808080606030302020201 04 07 13 09 13 20 16 07 16 29 06 09 10 50 312 11 2724667431 1124111221 4141211122111122211112121221211111122 1010101010101010101010101010101004040401 28 37 50 50 35 04 21 06 01 09 23 21 11 15 111112300145 19613 43124 2536353263 5323525566 2132311121123121121112121231211111232 1010090909080808070707060606050504040402 40 01 33 29 35 09 33 40 07 19 43 12 19 23 132 31 3536533353 5433535555 2132311121123121121112121231211111232 1010101009090908080808070707070606060505 50 50 50 10 50 26 05 18 50 06 50 50 25 10 111212210134 19613 31222 2635544423 2224434433 2121311121112111121112131221111211211 1010101010101010100908070505040303020101 26 50 03 30 37 04 22 24 08 04 18 12 08 06 213 33 2535444423 3334434433 2121211121112111121112121221111211121 1010101010101009090808080807060605040303 17 02 06 11 ll 19 06 07 11 50 24 06 50 25 111112301124 18713 11123 3525356665 2222313134 1111211122112111111112131231211212124 1010090808070505040403030202010101010101 50 01 50 16 31 18 50 33 35 50 10 11 10 08 132 12 3424356665 2222312134 1111111122112111111211111121211111122 1007070605040301010101010101010101010101 50 17 07 31 38 14 50 31 25 50 02 50 21 41 111111200131 22642 53214 4222464625 2112211353 2111211121113112111112131121111213221 1010090909090907070504040403030202020201 27 50 04 44 39 21 14 12 04 22 21 05 31 14 123 21 4422364635 3212213452 2111111121112112211112131111111213221 0909090909090806060606060605040404030303 07 11 07 14 17 09 14 10 03 16 12 02 25 21 111111510135 18613 31124 2233433433 2232333323 2122321232113123322113131231111313222 1010101010101010100909080807070706060504 47 04 04 12 50 50 22 50 41 50 03 23 03 41 123 21 ‘2533333422 2233333322 173 24 06 17 45 22 15 27 07 07 37 48 20 10 27 15 04 35 16 10 02 28 50 01 13 18 07 15 11 44 ll 24 12 25 29 05 15 45 25 27 10 06 25 19 43 11 11 22 50 26 11 24 35 50 25 04 12 13 35 09 10 16 07 06 50 36 33 11 09 13 25 50 09 13 05 13 23 50 50 11 50 50 21 11 15 8086 8087 8088 8089 8121 8122 8123 8124 8125 8126 8127 8128 8129 9011 9012 9013 9014 9015 9016 9017 9018 9019‘ 9101 9102 9103 9104 9105 9106 9107 9108 91093 9271 9272 9273 9274 9275 9276 9277 9278 9279 9541 9542 9543 9544 9545 9546 9547 9548 9549? 2122311132113124422212121231111212222 1010101010101010100909090908080807070706 29 16 12 18 15 07 19 14 12 20 24 27 37 50 111115453145 21633 53111 2453664643 3236315353 2111331121113111111112141111111311111 1010101010101009080707060605050502020101 50 48 34 43 44 22 16 20 29 50 27 41 35 19 123 32 3453662653 3366215453 1111311121112111111111131111111311111 1010101010090908080707070604040302010101 50 39 50 32 10 50 08 12 23 15 11 06 17 50 111113210131 21632 52124 3513177414 1111111111 1121311121113111121111111211211311212 1010090908060605050504040302020202020202 50 44 23 24 18 01 08 04 11 26 25 03 16 44 231 13 3513167414 1111111111 1121421131113111121111131211211311312 0909090808070707060505040403020202010101 43 25 38 08 04 16 46 08 46 33 25 22 17 35 111113423135 19611 31123 3525373334 3334324444 1111311131113111111111131111111311211 1010090909090909080808070706060605040403 50 45 30 35 36 25 46 43 40 35 25 15 06 01 231 23 3525374324 3333343344 1111311131112111111111131111111211211 1009090807070707060606050505040404030201 33 39 14 28 04 16 27 31 44 17 12 31 02 49 111113321133 18623 21123 3354231445 4344435455 1321411131112111111211141111111211311 0909090808070707060606060605050505040403 50 50 50 50 50 46 38 45 50 41 18 50 50 44 231 22 4363231455 4444445455 1111211121112112111111121211211211211 0707060505050404040303030303020201010101 44 50 35 41 32 35 44 14 50 46 33 25 38 34 111112200131 20233 21122 2637543414 3331424365 2141111111112111131112111321211111112 1009090909080808070606050504040403020201 31 22 19 29 40 33 08 03 20 50 42 22 42 09 231 23 2637553414 3332333265 2141111121111112121112111211211111112 0808080707060605050505040404040404030202 40 39 17 08 02 16 33 47 41 26 22 12 44 28 111113300133 174 50 31 20 33 38 11 25 16 25 12 16 23 37 12 13 14 21 22 34 45 19 10 50 29 07 18 08 20 13 22 31 44 07 14 19 13 41 05 38 28 43 17 45 50 22 13 50 38 11 25 39 17 50 17 23 27 50 50 24 19 18 24 24 50 11 39 LIST OF REFERENCES LIST OF REFERENCES Abelson, R. (April, 1975). Script processing in attitude formation and decision making. Paper presented at the Eleventh Carnegie- Mellon Symposium on Cognition. Alba, J. W., & Hasher, L. (1983). Is memory schematic? Psychological Bulletin, fl, 203—231. Albrecht, R. R., & Ewing, S. J. (1989). Standardizing the administration of the Profile of Mood States (POMS): Development of alternative word lists. Journal of Personaligg Assessment, fl, 31-39. Albrecht, R. R., & Feltz, D. L. (1987). Generality and specificity of attention related to competitive anxiety and sport performance. Journal of Sport Psychology, 9, 231-248. American Psychological Association (1988). Thesaurus of psychological index terms. (5th Ed.). Washington, D.C.: Author. Anderson, J ., 81 Bower, G. (1973). Human associative memogg. Washington, DC: Winston. Anshel, M. H. (1987). The effect of mood and pleasant versus unpleasant information feedback on performing a motor skill. Journal of General Psycholog, 1 15, 1 17-129. Asher, H. B. (1983). Causal modeling. Sage University Paper series on Quantitative Applications in the Social Sciences, series no. 07- 003. Bahrke, M., Thompson, C.. & Thomas, T. (1986). Mood alterations following aerobic exercise withdrawal. Indiana Alliance for Health, Physical Education, Recreation and Dance Journal, fie), 8-10. Baldwin, M. W., Carrell, S. E., & Lopez, D. F. (1990). Priming relationship schemas: My advisor and the Pope are watching me from the back of my mind. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 2Q, 435-454. Baldwin, M. W., & Holmes, J. G. (1987). Salient private audiences and awareness of the self. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 5_3, 1087-1098. Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. Psychological Review, 8_4, 191-215. 175 176 Bandura, A. (1982). Self-efficacy in human agency. Cognitive Therapy and Research, _1_, 287-308. Bandura, A. (1986). Spcial fpundatipns pf thpught app action: A social cognitive thegry. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Bandura, A., Adams, N. E., 81 Beyer, J. (1977). Cognitive processes mediating behavioral change. Journal of Personality and Social Psyphglogg, ,1; 125-139. Bargh, J. A. (1982). Attention and automaticity in the processing of self-relevant information. Journal of Pgrsonality apd Social Psychplogz, 43, 425-436. Bargh, J. A., 81 Peitromonaco, P. (1982). Automatic information processing and social perception: The influence of trait information presented outside of conscious awareness on impression formation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychologg, 411, 437-449. Barling, J ., 81 Abel, M. (1983). Self-efficacy beliefs and tennis performance. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 1, 265-272. Baron, R. S. (1986). Distraction-conflict theory: Progress and problems. Advances in Experimental Sppial Psychology, fl, 1-40. Bartlett, F. C. (1932). Remembering. London: Cambridge University Press. Beck, A. T. (1967). Depression: Clinical, experimental. and theoretical aspects. New York: Harper 81 Row. Beck, A. T. (1976). Cognitive therapy and the emotional disorders. New York: International Universities Press. Berger, G., 81 Owen, R. (1988). Stress reduction and mood enhancement in four exercise modes: Swimming, body conditioning, hatha yoga, and fencing. Research Quarterly for Exercise Science and Sport, 5_9, 148-159. Berkowitz, L. (1974). Some determinants of impulsive aggression: Role of mediated associations with reinforcements for aggression. Psychologipal Revigw, &, 165- 176. Berkowitz, L., 81 Alioto, J. T. (1973). The meaning of an observed event as a determinant of its aggressive consequences. Journal of Pgrsonality and Social Psychology, 18, 206-217. 177 Berkowitz, L., 81 Green, R. G. (1967). Stimulus qualities of the target of aggression: A further study. Journal of Personality and Social Psycholoa, 5, 364-368. Berkowitz, L., 81 Rogers, K. H. (1986). A priming effect analysis of media influences. In J. Bryant 81 D. Zillmann (Eds.) Perspectives on media effects (pp. 57-81). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Bird, A. M., 81 Horn, M. A. (1990). Cognitive anxiety and mental errors in sport. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 1_2, 217-222. Blackman, J ., 81 Homstein, H. (1977). Newscasts and the social actuary. Public Opinion Quarterly, fl, 295-313. Bond, C. F., Jr. (1982). Social facilitation: A self—presentational View. Journal of Personaligg and Social Psychology, fl, 1042-1050. Bower, G. H. (1981). Mood and memory. American Psychologist, &, 129-148. Bransford, J. D., 81 Johnson, M. K. (1972). Contextual prerequisites for understanding: Some investigations of comprehension and recall. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, Q, 717 — 726. Bray, J. H., 81 Maxwell, S. E. (1985). Multivarate analysis of van'ance. Sage University Paper series on Quantitative Applications in the Social Sciences, series no 07—054. Beverly Hills: Sage Publications. Breslow, R., Kocsis, J .. 81 Belkin, B. (1981). Contribution of the depressive perspective to memory function in depression. American Journal of Psychiatgg, 138, 227-230. Brewer, W. F., 81 Nakamura, G. V. (1984). The nature and function of schemas. In R. S. Wyer, Jr. 81 T. K. Srull (Eds.), Handbook of social cognition (Vol. 1, pp. 119—160). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Broadbent, D. E. (1970). Frederic Charles Bartlett: 1886-1969. Biographical Memoirs of Fellows of the Royal Society, _1_§, 1-13. Bruch, M., Chesser, E. S., 81 Meyer, V. (1989). The role of evaluative self-schemata in cognitive processing and performance: The impact of self—efficacy, self-evaluation and task outcome. Scandinavian Journal of Behaviour Therapy, E, 71-84. 178 Bruch, M., Meyer, V., 81 Chesser, E. S. (1987). The role of evaluative self-schemata in self—talk: Some predictions and explorations. Scandinavian Journal of Behaviour Therapy, _1_7, 149-166. Bruner, J. S. (1957). On perceptual readiness. Psychological Review, _6_4, 123-152. Bruner, J. S. (1958). Social psychology and perception. In E. E. Maccoby, T. M. Newcomb, 81 E .L. Hartley (Eds.), Readings in social psycholog (pp. 85—94). New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston. Cantor, N., & Mishel, W. (1977). Traits as prototypes: Effects on recognition memory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, &, 38-48. Carver, C. S., Ganellen, R. J ., Froming, W. J ., 81 Chambers, W. (1983). Modeling: An analysis in terms of category accessibility. Journal of Experimental Social Psycholoaz, fl, 403-424. Clark, D. M., 81 Teasdale, J. D. (1982). Diurnal variation of clinical depression and accessibility of memories of positive and negative experiences. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 9_1, 87-95. Clark, D. M., 81 Teasdale, J. D. (1985). Constraints on the effects of mood and memory. Journal of Personalng and Social Psychology, 48, 1595-1608. Corbin, C. B., Laurie, D. R., Gruger, C., 81 Smiley, B. (1984). Vicarious success experience as a factor influencing self-confidence, attitudes and physical activity of adult women. Journal of Teaching of Physical Education, 4, 17-23. Cottrell, N. B. (1972). Social Facilitation. In C. G. McClintock (Ed.), Experimental social psycholog. New York: Holt, Rinehart 81 Winston. Craighead, D., Privette, G., Vallianos, F., 81 Byrkit, D. (1986). Personality characteristics of basketball players, starters and non- starters. International Journal of Sport Psychologz, fl, 1 10-1 19. Cronbach, L. J. (1951). Coefficient alpha and the internal structure of tests. Psychometrika,1_6, 297-334. Crowne, D. P., 81 Marlowe, D. (1960). A new scale of social desirability independent of psychopathology. Journal of Consulting Psychologg, 2_4, 349-354. 179 Daiss, S., LeUnes, A., 81 Nation, J. R. (1986). Mood and locus of control of college and professional football players. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 83, 733-734. Daus, A., Wilson, J ., 81 Freeman, W. (1986). Psychological testing as an auxiliary means of selecting successful college and professional football players. Journal of Sports Medicine and Physical Fitness, 28, 274-278. Deshamais, R., Bouillon, J ., 81 Godin, G. (1986). Self-efficacy and outcome expectations as determinants of exercise adherence. Psychological Reports, 5_9, 1155-1159. Edwards, S., 81 Huston, S. (1984). The clinical aspects of sport psychology. Physical Educator, 4_1, 142-148. Endler, N. S. (1973). The person versus the situation -- A pseudo issue? A response to Alker. Journal of Personality, 41, 287—303. Endler, N. S. (1975). The role of person by situation interaction in personality theory. In C. D. Spielberger 81 I. G. Sarason (Eds.), Stress and anxiety (Vol. 1, pp. 145-164). Washington, DC: Hemisphere. Farrell, P., Gates, W., Maksud, M., 81 Morgan, W. P. (1982). Increases in Plasma B-endorphin/B-lipotropin immunoreactivity after treadmill running in humans. Journal of Applied Psychology, Q, 1245-1249. Feltz, D. L. (1982). Path analysis of the causal elements in Bandura's theory of self-efficacy and an anxiety—based model of avoidance behavior. Journal of Personalig; and Social Psychology, g, 764- 781. Feltz, D. L. (1988a). Gender differences in the causal elements of self- efficacy on a high avoidance motor task. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, m, 151-166. Feltz, D. L. (1988b). Self-confidence and sports performance. In K. Pandolf (Ed.), Exercise and sport science reviews (Vol. 16, pp. 423-457). New York: Macmillan. Feltz, D. L. (1992). Understanding motivation in sport: A self-efficacy perspective. In G. C. Roberts (Ed.), Motivation in sport and exercise (pp. 93-105). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics. 180 Feltz, D. L., 81 Albrecht, R. R. (1986). The influence of self-efficacy on the approach/ avoidance of a high-avoidance motor task. In L. Vander Velden 81 J. Humphrey (Eds.), Psychology and sociolofl of sport: Current selected research (Vol. 1, pp. 3-25). New York: AMS Press. Feltz, D. L., Landers, D. M., 81 Raeder, U. (1979). Enhancing self- eflicacy in high-avoidance motor tasks: A comparison of modeling techniques. Journal of Sport Psychology, _1_, 1 12-122. Feltz, D. L., Marcotullio, S., 81 Fitzgerald, C. (1985, May). The effects of different forms of in vivo emotive imagegg on self-efficacy and competitive endurance performance. Paper presented at the North American Society for the Psychology of Sport and Physical Activity, Gulfport, MS. Feltz, D. L., 81 Mugno, D. A. (1983). A replication of the path analysis of the causal elements in Bandura's theory of self-efficacy and the influence of autonomic perception. Journal of Sport Psycholog, 5, 263-277. Feltz, D. L., 81 Riessinger, C. A. (1990). Effects of in vivo emotive imagery and performance feedback on self-efficacy and muscular endurance. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychologg, _1_2_, 132- 143. Fiske, S. T., 81 Taylor, S. E. (1984). Social cognition. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley. Forgas, J. P., Bower, G. H., 81 Krantz, S. E. (1984). The influence of mood on perceptions of social interactions. Journal of Experimental Social Psychologl, @, 497-513. Freedson, P., Mihevic, P., Loucks, A., 81 Girandola, R. (1983). Physique, body composition, and psychological characteristics of competitive female body builders. Physician and Sportsmedicine, 11(5), 85-93. Fuchs, C., 81 Zaichkowsky, L. (1983). Psychological characteristics of male and female body builders: The iceberg profile. Journal of Sport Behavior, 8, 136-145. Gardner, H. (1985). The mind's new science: A histoty of the cognitive revolution. New York: Basic Books Inc. Gayton, W. F., Matthews, G. R., 81 Burchstead, G. N. (1986). An investigation of the validity of the Physical Self-Efficacy Scale in 181 predicting marathon performance. Perceptug ape Metor Skille, fiat 752-754. Ghiselli, E. E., Campbell, J. P., 81 Zedeck, S. (1981). Measurement theegg fer me behavioral sciences. San Francisco: Freeman Gould, D., 81 Weiss, M. (1981). The effects of model similarity and model talk on self-efficacy and muscular endurance. Journal of 5mm. 3. 17-29. Greenwald, A. G., 81 Pratkanis. A. R. (1984). The self. In R. S. Wyer, Jr. 81 T. K. Srull (Eds.), Hepdpeek of eeeie1 eognition, (Vol. 3, pp. 129-178). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Gutmann, M., Pollock, M., Foster, 0., 81 Schmidt, D. (1984). Training stress in Olympic speed skaters: A psychological perspective. Physicifl Ed Sportemedieine, Q02), 45-57. Guyton, A. C. (1981). Textbook of medical h siolo 6th Ed. . Philadelphia: W. B. Saunders. Hagberg, J ., Mullin, J. Bahrke, M., 81 Limberg, J. (1979). Physiological profiles and selected psychological characteristics of national class American cyclists. Journal of Sports Medieine, 19, 341-346. Hall, H. K., 81 Byrne, A. T. J. (1988). Goal setting in sport: Clarifying recent anomalies. Journal of Spogt and Exercise Psychology, 1Q, 184-198. Head, H. (1918). Sensation and the cerebral cortex. Brain, 41, 57- 253. Head, H. (1926). Aphasia and kindred disorders of speech. Cambridge. Henschen, K., Horvat, M., 81 French, R. (1984). A visual comparison of psychological profiles between able-bodied and wheelchair athletes. Adapted Physical Activity Quarterly, 1, 1 18-124. Herr, P. M. (1986). Consequences of priming: Judgment and behavior. Journal of Pereonality Qd Social Psychology, 51, 1 106- 1 1 1 5. Herr, P. M., Sherman, S. J., 81 Fazio, R. H. (1983). On the consequences of priming: Assimilation and contrast effects. Jeurnal of Experimental Social Psychology, 19, 323-340. 182 Higgins, E. T., Bargh, J. A., 81 Lombardi, W. (1985). Nature of priming effects on categorization. Journal of Qpefimental Social thelegy: gaming, Memery and Qognition, 1_1_, 59-69. Higgins, E. T., 81 King, G. (1981). Accessibility of social constructs: Information processing consequences of individual and contextual variability. In N. Cantor 81 J. F. Kihlstrom (Eds.), Beam cognition and social interaction, (pp. 69-121). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Higgins, E. T., King, G. A., 81 Mavin, G. H. (1982). Individual construct accessibility and subjective impressions and recall. J ournfl of Persenality and Social Psychologz, 4_3, 35-47. Higgins, E. T., Rholes, W. S., 81 Jones, C. R. (1977). Category accessibility and impression formation. Journal of Experimental flcial Psychology, 13, 1 4 l - 1 54. Hogan, P. I., 81 Santomier, J. P. (1984). Effect of mastering swim skills on older adults' self-efficacy. Research uarterl for Exercise Science and Sport, fl, 294-296. Holloway, 5., Tucker, L., 81 Homstein, H. (1977). The effects of social and nonsocial information on interpersonal behavior of males: The news makes news. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, _3_5, 514-522. Homstein, H., LaKind, E., Frankel, G., 81 Manne, S. (1975). Effects of knowledge about remote social events on prosocial behavior, social conception and mood. Journal of Personality and Social Peychology, _3_2, 1038-1046. Isen, A. M., Shalker, T. E., Clark, M.. 81 Karp, L. (1978). Affect, accessibility of material in memory, and behavior: A cognitive loop? Journal of Personaligg and Social Psychology, 38, 1-12. Jensen, B. E. (1978). Introduction to research in physical education. Springfield College: Springfield, MA. Johnson, A., Collins, P., Higgins, 1., Harrington, D.,'Connolly, J., Dolphin, C., McCreery, M., Brady, L., 81 O'Brien, M. ( 1985). Psychological, nutritional and physical status of Olympic road cyclists. Journal of Spogts Medieine and Physical Fitness, 19, 11- 14. Johnson, W. A., 81 Dark, V. J. (1986). Selective attention. Annual Review of Psychology, 31, 43-75. 183 Josephson, W. (1981). Television violence and children's aggression: Dieinhipitten, elieitatien er eathgsie? Paper presented to the Canadian Psychological msociation. Kavanagh, D. J ., 81 Bower, G. H. (1985). Mood and self-efficacy: Impact of joy and sadness on perceived capabilities. Qegnitive Therapy and Research, _9_, 507-525. Kavanagh, D. J ., 81 Hausfeld, S. (1986). Physical performance and self- eflicacy under happy and sad moods. Journal of Spert Psychelegy, 5! 1 12-123. Kelley, H. H. (1950). The warm-cold variable in first impressions of persons. 510qu of Personality, 1S, 431-439. Kendzierski, D. J. (1988). Self-schemata and exercise. Basic and Applied Social Psycholog, e, 45-59. Kendzierski, D. J. (1990). Exercise self-schemata: Cognitive and behavioral correlates. Health Psycholoaz, 9, 69-82. Kihlstrom, J. F., 81 Cantor, N. (1984). Mental representations of the self. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social peychologg (Vol. 17, pp. 1-47). New York: Academic Press. Kowal, D., Patton, J ., 81 Vogel, J. (1978). Psychological states and aerobic fitness of male and female recruits before and after basic training. Aviation Space Ed Envirenmentg Medieine, 19, 603- 606. Kubovy, M. (1977). Response availability and the apparent spontaneity of numerical choices. Journal ef Experimental Psychology: Human Pereeption and Performance, S, 359-364. Kuiper, N. A., MacDonald, M. R., & Derry, P. A. (1983). Parameters of a depressive self-schema. In J. Suls 81 A. G. Greenwald (Eds.), Psychological perspectives of the self (Vol. 2, pp. 191-217). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Lee, C. (1982). Self-efficacy as a predictor of performance in competitive gmnastics. Journg of Sport Psyehology, _4_, 405-409. LeUnes, A., Hayward, S. A., 81 Daiss, S. (1989). Annotated bibliography on the Profile of Mood States in sport, 1975-1988. Journal of Sport Behavior, fl. 213-239. 184 Lichtrnan, S., 81 Posner, E. (1983). The effects of exercise on mood and cognitive functioning. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 21, 43-52. Locke, E. A. (1991). Problems with goal-setting research in sports -- and their solution. Journal of Spog; Ed Exereiee Psychology, 13, 3 1 1 -3 16. Lombardi, W., Higgins, E. T., 81 Bargh, J. A. (1987). The role of consciousness in priming effects on categorization: Assimilation versus contrast as a function of the priming task. Pereenality and cial P holo Bull tin, 13, 411-429. Loyd, G. G., 81 Lishman, W. A. (1975). Effect of depression on the speed of recall of pleasant and unpleasant experiences. Peychologieat Medicine, 5, 173-180. McAuley, E. (1985). Modeling and self-efficacy: A test of Bandura's model. Journal of Sport Psyehology, 1, 283-295. McAuley. E., 81 Gill, D. (1983). Reliability and validity of the Physical Self-Efficacy Scale in a competitive sport setting. Jeurnal ef Sport Psycholoa, S, 410-418. McGowan, R. W., 81 Jordan, C. D. (1988). Mood states and physical activity. Louisiana Alliance for Health. Physical Education. Recreation and Dance Journal, 15(2), 12-13, 17, 32. McNair, D. M., Lorr, M., 81 Droppleman, L. F. (1971). Mapual for the Profile ef Meod States. San Diego: Educational 81 Industrial Testing Service. Magill, R. A. (1985). Motor learning concepts and applications. Dubuque, IA: Wm. C. Brown. Mahoney, M. J. (1989). Psychological predictors of elite and non-elite performance in olympic weightlifting. International Journal of Sport Psycholog, @, l-12. Markus, H. (1977). Self-schemata and processing information about the self. Journal of Personality Ed Secial Psychology, S5, 63-78. Markus, H., Crane, M., 81 Siladi, M. (May, 1978). Qognitive consequences of androgymy. Paper presented to the Midwestern Psychological Association, Chicago. 185 Markus, H., 81 Sentis, K. (1982). The self in social information processing. In J. Suls (Ed.), Psychological perspectives on the self (Vol. 1, pp. 41-70). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Markus, H., 81 Smith, J. (1981). The influence of self-schemata on the perception of others. In N. Cantor 81 J. Kihlstrom (Eds.), Persenafity, cognition Ed eocial interaction. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum Associates. Martin, L. L. (1986). Set/reset: Use and disuse of concepts in impression formation. Jeurnal ef Personality and Social Peyeholegy, 5_1, 493-504. Meyers, M., Sterling, J ., 81 LeUnes, A. (1988). Psychological characterization of the collegiate rodeo athlete. Journal of Sport Behavior, 11, 59-65. Milgram, S. (1963). Behavioral study of obedience. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, _61, 371-378. Miller, B., 81 Miller, A. (1985). Psychological correlates of success in elite sportswomen. International Jeurnal of Spor_'t Psychology, m, 289-295. Minsky, M. (1975). A framework for representing knowledge. In P. H. Winston, (Ed.), The psycholegy of computer vision. New York: McGraw-Hill. Morgan, W. P. (1980a, July). Test of champions. Psychology Today, pp. 92-99. Morgan, W. P. (1980b). The trait psychology controversy. Research Qpageglz for Exercise and Spott, 51, 50-76. Morgan, W. P., Brown, D. R., Raglin, J. S., O'Connor, P. J ., 81 Ellickson, K. A. (1987). Psychological monitoring of overtraining and staleness. British Journal of Sports Medicine, 21, 107-1 14. Morgan, W. P., 81 Horstman, D. (1978). Psychometric correlates of pain perception. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 41, 27 -39. Morgan, W. P., 81 Johnson, R. (1978). Personality characteristics of successful and unsuccessful oarsmen. International Journal of Sport Psychology, a, 119-133. Morgan, W. P., O'Connor, P. J ., Ellickson, K. A., 81 Bradley, P. W. (1988). Personality structure, mood states, and performance in 186 elite male distance runners. Internatienal Jeurnal ef Speg Psychologz, 111, 247-263. Morgan, W. P., 81 Pollock, M. L. (1977). Psychologic characterization of the elite distance runner. Annals of me New York Academy of m. &1. 382-403. Nagle, F., Morgan, W. P., Hellickson, R., Serfass, R., 81 Alexander, J. (1975). Spotting success traits in Olympic contenders. Physieian @g Spgrtemegieine, 3(12), 31—34, 84. Natale, M., 81 Hantas, M. (1982). Effect of temporary mood states on selective memory about the self. Journal of Pereenality and Social Psychology, 4_1, 927-934. Nation, J. R., 81 LeUnes, A. (1981, September). Semi-tough and supernormal. Psychology Today, pp. 66-67. Neisser, U. (1976). Cognition and reality: Prineiples and implications of cognitive psychology. San Francisco: Freeman. Nideffer, R. M. (1976). The inner athlete: Mind plue muscle for winning. New York: Crowell. Oldfield, R. C. (1972). Frederic Charles Bartlett: 1886-1969. American Journal of Psychology, SS, 133-140. Pedhazur, E. J. (1982). Multiple regression in behavioral research: Explanation and prediction. New York: Holt, Rinehart 81 Winston. Petty, R. E., 81 Cacioppo, J. T. (1986). The Elaboration Likelihood Model of persuasion. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimengl social psycholog (Vol. 19, pp. 124-206). Orlando, FL: Academic Press. Poole, R., 81 Henschen, K. (1984). Brigham Young's psychological program for women's cross-country and track. Scholastic Coach, 51(8), 52-53, 73-74, 77. Pragman, D. (1986). Stress and motor performance: Understanding Ed coping. Ithaca, NY: Mouvement. Ratcliff, R. 81 McKoon, G. (1988). A retrieval theory of priming in memory. Psychological Review, SS, 385-408. Raven, B. H., 81 Rubin, J. z. (1976). Social peychologg: People in groups. New York: Wiley. 187 Reynolds, W. M. (1982). Development of reliable and valid short forms of the Marlowe-Crowne social desirability scale. J eumg pf glinical Peycholegy, S8, 1 19-125. Riddick, C. (1984). Comparative psychological profiles of three groups of female collegians: Competitive swimmers, recreational swimmers and inactive swimmers. Journal of Sport Behavior, 1, 160-174. Riskind, J. H. (1989). The mediating mechanisms’in mood and memory: A cognitive-priming formulation. In D. Kuiken (Ed.), Mood and memory: Theory, research and applications [Special Issue]. Journal of Social Behavior and Personaligg, _4, 173-184. Robinson, D., 81 Howe, B. (1987). Causal attribution and mood state relationships of soccer players in a sport achievement setting. Jeurnal ef Sport Behavior, 1Q, 137-146. Rumelhart, D. E. (1975). Notes on a schema for stories. In D. G. Bobrow 81 A. Collins (Eds.). Representation and understanding. New York: Academic Press. Rumelhart, D. E. (1981). Understanding understanding. University of California San Diego Center for Human Information Processing, Technical Report 100. Rumelhart, D. E. (1984). Schemata and the cognitive system. In R. S. Wyer, Jr. 81 T. K. Srull, (Eds.), Handbook of social cognition (Vol. 1, pp. 161-188). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Rumelhart, D. E., 81 Ortony, A. (1977). The representation of knowledge in memory. In R. C. Anderson, R. J. Spiro, 81 W. E. Montague (Eds.), Schooling and the acquisition of knowledge. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Ryckman, R. M., Robbins, M. A., Thornton, B., 81 Cantrell, P. (1982). Development and validation of a physical self-efficacy scale. Jeurnal ef Personality and Social Psychology, 12, 891-900. Schachter, S., 81 Singer, J. E. (1962). Cognitive, social and physiological determinants of emotional state. Psychological Review, fl, 379—399. Schmidt, R. A. (1975). A schema theory of discrete motor skill learning. Peyehological Review, S2, 225-260. 188 Sentis, K., 81 Markus, H. (1979). Self-eehemae apt! reeegnigen memog. Unpublished manuscript, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI. Shacham, S. (1983). A shortened version of the Profile of Mood States. Jeurnal ef Pereonaflty Assessment, fl, 305-306. Silva, J. M., & Weinberg, R. S. (1984). Psychological foundations of eppxt. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics. Simon, J. R. (1988). A "priming" effect in a choice reaction time task. Aeta Peycholegica, Q, 45-60. Singer, J. L., 81 Salovey, P. (1991). Organized knowledge structures and personality: Person schemas, self schemas, prototypes, and scripts. In M. J. Horowitz (Ed.), Person echemae and maladaptive integpersonal patterns. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Skelton, J. A., 81 Strohmetz, D. B. (1990). Priming symptom reports with health-related cognitive activity. Personality and Social Peflhology Bulletin, 16, 449-464. Smith, E. R. (1988). Category accessibility effects in a simulated exemplar-based memory. Journal of Experimental Social W9 2_49 448-463. Smith, E. R., 81 Branscombe, N. R. (1987). Procedurally mediated social inferences: The case of category accessibility effects. Journal of Experimental Social Psychologg, E, 361—382. Smith, M., 81 Lee, C. (1992). Goal setting and performance in a novel coordination task: Mediating mechanisms. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, fl. 169-176. Smith, R. E., Smoll, F. L., 81 Hunt, E. (1977). A system for the behavioral assessment of athletic coaches. Research Quarterly, 18, 401-407. Spray, J. A. (1986). Absolute error revisited: An accuracy indicator in disguise. Journal of Motor Behavior. 1_8, 225-238. Srull, T. K., 81 Wyer, R. 3., Jr. (1979). The role of category accessibility in the interpretation of information about persons: Some determinants and implications. Journal of Personality and Seeial Psyeholofl, .31. 1660-1672. 189 Srull, T. K., 81 Wyer, R. S., Jr. (1980). Category accessibility and social perception: Some implications for the study of person memory and interpersonal judgments. Journal of Personality and Social Psyehelogy, 38. 841-856. Stanley, M. A., 81 Maddux, J. E. (1986). Self-efficacy expectancy and depressed mood: An investigation of causal relationships. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 1, 575-586. Sweeney, P., 81 Morland, R. L. (1980). Self-schemas and the perseverance of beliefs about the self. Paper presented to the American Psychological Association, Montreal. - Taylor, S. E., 81 Crocker, J. (1981). Schematic bases of social information processing. In E. T. Higgins, C. P. Herman, 81 M. P. Zanna (Eds.), Social cognition: The Qntario Smposium (Vol. 1, pp. 89-133). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Teasdale, J. D., 81 Fogarty, S. J. (1979). Differential effects of induced mood on retrieval of pleasant and unpleasant events from episodic memory. Journal of Abnormal Psychologz, _8_8, 248-257. Tharion, W. J ., Strowman, S. R., 81 Ranch, T. M. (1988). Profile and changes in moods of ultramarathoners. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 1Q, 229-235. Thaxton, L. (1982). Physiological and psychological effects of short- term exercise addiction on habitual runners. Journal of Sport Psychology, 1, 7 3-80. Van Schoyck, S. R., 81 Grasha, A. F. (1981). Attentional style variations and athletic ability: The advantages of a sports-specific test. Jeurnal of Sport Psychology. 3.. 149-165. Velten, E. (1968). A laboratory task for induction of mood states. Behavior Research and Therapy, 6, 473-482. Wann, D. L., 81 Branscombe, N. R. (1990). Person perception when aggressive or nonaggressive sports are primed. Aggressive Behavior, 16_, 27-32. Weinberg, R. (1986). Relationship between self-efficacy and cognitive strategies in enhancing endurance performance. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 1_7, 280-293. 190 Weinberg, R., Gould, D., 81 Jackson, A. (1979). Expectations and performance: An empirical test of Bandura's self-efficacy theory. Journal ef Spott Peychelog‘y, 1, 320-331. Weinberg, R. S., Gould, D., Yukelson, D., 81 Jackson, A. (1981). The effect of preexisting and manipulated self-efficacy on a competitive muscular endurance task. Journal ef Spogt Psychologg, 1, 345-354. Weinberg, R. S., Sinardi, M., 81 Jackson, A. (1982). Effect of bar height and modeling on anxiety, self-confidence and gymnastic performance. International Gmnast, a, 11-13. Weinberg, R. S., Yukelson, D., 81 Jackson, A. (1980). Effect of public and private efficacy expectations on competitive performance. Journal of Sport Psychologz, _2_, 340-349. Wilfley, D., 81 Kunce, J. (1986). Differential physical and psychological effects of exercise. Journal of Counseling Psychologz, 3_3, 337- 342. Wilkes, R. L., 81 Summers, J. J. (1984). Cognitions, mediating variables, and strength performance. Jeurnal of Sport Psyehologz, 6. 351-359. Wilson, V., Ainsworth, M., 81 Bird, E. (1985). Assessment of attentional abilities in male volleyball athletes. International Journal of Sport Psychologz, 16, 296-306. Woolfolk, R. L., Murphy, S. M., Gottesfeld, D., 81 Aitken, D. (1985). Effects of mental rehearsal of task motor activity and mental depiction of task outcome on motor skill performance. Journal of Spert Peyeholegy, 1. 191-197. Worchel, S. (1981). The effect of films on the importance of behavioral freedom. Journal of Personality, 1Q, 417-435. Wright, J ., 81 Mischel, W. (1982). The influence of affect on cognitive social learning person variables. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43,, 901—914. Wyer, R. S., Jr., 81 Gordon, S. E. (1984). The cognitive representation of social information. In R. S. Wyer, Jr. 81 T. K. Srull (Eds.), Handbook of social cognition (Vol. 2, pp. 73-150). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. 191 Wyer, R. 8., Jr., 81 Srull, T. K. (1980). The processing of social stimulus information: A conceptual integration. In R. Hastie, T. Ostrom, E. Ebbesen, R. Wyer, Jr., D. Hamilton 81 D. Carlston (Eds.), Pereen memergy: The eegnip’ve baeie ef eeeial pereeptlon (pp. 227-300). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Wyer, R. 8., Jr., 81 Srull, T. K. (1981). Category accessibility: Some theoretical and empirical issues concerning the processing of social stimulus information. In E. Higgins, C. Herman, 81 M. Zanna (Eds.), Social eognition: The Ontg‘o Symposium (Vol. 1, pp. 161- 197). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Wyer, R. S., Jr., 81 Srull, T. K. (1986). Human cognition in its social context. Psychological Review, fl, 322-359. Yan Lan, L., 81 Gill, D. L. (1984). The relationships among self-efficacy, stress responses, and a cognitive feedback manipulation. Journal of Sport Psychology, S, 227-238. Zajonc, R. B. (1965). Social facilitation. Science, 149, 269-274. Zangwfll, O. L. (1972). Remembering revisited. Quarterly Journal ef Experimental Psychology, £15 123—138. ll HICHIGQ . 1 .. 331? .31.... .t .1. 13...: .. c. n. _. runwmmwwew _. .33..