sausémflkré. {Eu . _ .2. it. I... ‘.Yl {Sir I :1 V , ._ .6555»... manna...» a... .m... .r: . . ‘ fit , , a... . . V . i: . 6N5... . V .1 5.... . i! -‘ c :3 .1 {iii-n . .9. .4”. .ul‘oaasnb...&. . :. flan»... . .arm. a .9??? if u s ,. J... . s . n... .. :3. l . R 35725}: 55“. . I ~ (.Sxacr7 . . $.34... . x L“... .z 155...... . . .. fink. .5. . 5...... sh“... ’16.. tn. 4...: . . . t .34.. at“ n41”... flu... . . :iatv: . .. , . 3...... . . . I . , . .mmfiu... . . . Lu. 3.25.... 530.519.: 31' u n. . nu . .3 .. z. . , 3...... ,5..." 9......_m_e...... . ...t....._ 3...»... .... ,Juv .. .31... :5... .3... ; 1. .,' .3 r 5...... x x.vl: c 52... it... .. 511A. .... 1!. .3 t..!..v.: rvllu. 3......25. L... i............. l): n... H.432. , u”; .2 . n x .3... r .22.... 3...}... .2. . .....!.....L .. 3. 4..-...5: (on..- in}... . . . . . g u, 15...... . . ., ...Y..;....._...Fa . .al‘ifiquvlai’—vp A x..........!.... . Avtzr. . I “It...” .5 .. Hum?“ {1.0“}.le t... 11...}. 4.3.1:... .3. - X}... .... 2.... 3.... L a‘. ‘31! ’rll L 33.. .v. .hoo.!?!.¢.. .. ......m.. 2.. .il. )1 1... 3.0.9. 14. L... v an”... .38.... .anlfiit... . . . in. {huhih tl p-59... . 1r... \.vcli!. 3. D5..l.. iii! ’le’ld tail... ‘1 al. I ‘x‘su Stud. a .1...“ (Fla-uh 7.5.] .u. a\ v. .‘5. .1,‘ ll! \9. i»..4..... 2.1.5... . $12.... $5.... I: .v. . I ‘l. O )1. - :ali . (.5. dgiinlofinv4 o: 7. > ‘55.? 2.": .\:.r!. :1. ubwmvw. : . . , . . 1v LIBRARIES . \\\\\l\l‘l\\\\\\\\l moment STATE UNNE \' l\\\\\\\\\\l\ \\\\\\\\l\\\\\l\‘ll1‘i 31293 01 5 3500 ,. ."I This is to certify that the thesis entitled THE EFFECTS OF PREDATOR MEDIATED HABITAT USE ON CONSUMER-RESOURCE INTERACTIONS presented by Andrew Mark Turner has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for PhoDo degree in 200108)? Mai/W1 Major professor Date 1 November 1994 0-7639 MS U is an Affirmative Action/Equal Opportunity Institution LIBRARY University Michigan $tate PLAcE ll RETURN ”Xmmwombmuhmmm. TOAVOiDFINESMunonorbdonddoduo. DATE DUE DATE DUE DATE DUE “E L IIIII IIC I k———-—————- MSUIOMWWWOMIW WM! THE EFFECTS OF PREDATOR MEDIATED HABITAT USE ON CONSUMER-RESOURCE INTERACTIONS By Andrew Mark Turner A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY WK. Kellogg Biological Station and Department of Zoology 1994 ABSTRACT THE EFFECTS OF PREDATOR MEDIATED HABITAT USE ON CONSUMER-RESOURCE INTERACTIONS By Andrew Mark Turner In addition to killing and consuming prey, predators alter the behavior of prey, and the indirect effects of predator-mediated behaviors may be strong and far-reaching. In this thesis I show that the freshwater snail 311%]?! gm alters its behavior when in risk of being preyed upon, and that this change in behavior can affect the abundance of the periphyton that snails feed on. EhyglLa use covered substrates more in lakes containing the molluscivorous pumpkinseed sunfish Lem m than in adjacent lakes lacking pumpkinseeds. I found that the survivorship of Mse—lla in experimental pools was much higher when covered habitats were available. Ma do not change their habitat use when exposed to the water that has been in contact with pumpkinseeds. However, when exposed to water that had been in contact with crushed conspecifics, M increased its refuge use two-fold over controls, demonstrating that P_hy&lla can perceive mortality risk and facultatively alter it's behavior in an adaptive manner. Habitat structure also affects the behavior of P_hysell_a. In experimental pools containing a covered habitat m respond to risk by moving under cover, while in pools lacking cover Mil; respond to risk by moving to the water's surface. In a second set of experiments I tested the hypothesis that the behavioral response of liaise—Ila to predation risk would influence periphyton standing crop. m responded to risk by abandoning open habitats, and periphyton standing crop in the open habitat was positively related to the level of predation risk on snails. However, the strength of the responses were time dependent. Early in the experiment predation risk had a strong effect on Ma habitat use, but a weak effect on periphyton standing crop. Late in the experiment predation risk had a weak effect on M habitat use, and a strong effect on periphyton standing crop. Finally, I present a theoretical framework that synthesizes these results and makes predictions amenable to experimental testing. This work is dedicated to the two people who taught me where to find the fish: my Grandparents, Woody and Rosemary Patton. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS First and foremost, I acknowledge the large role that the academic community at the Kellogg Biological Station has played in my graduate training. We learn best from the examples of our peers, and a Ph.D. thesis mirrors the institution from which it was created as much as the person who created it. The members of my committee were Gary Mittelbach, Tom Getty, Alan Tessier, Kay Gross, and Steve Tonsor; other KBS/MSU faculty who have influenced me include Lars Hedin, Susan Kalisz, Phil Robertson, Mike King, George Lauff, Don Hall, and Don Straney, and Bill Cooper. There is one person whose contributions to this work demand special acknowledgement. The demons that haunt every Ph.D. candidate fought me to a standstill, leaving me exhausted and confused, and the battle appeared lost. However, when the skies of my academic future were at their darkest, Wendy Reed had to courage to enter the battle and fight alongside me. Wendy reminded me by the force of her own example that strength of character can overcome all because, in the end, character is all that matters. Thank-you Wendy. The Lux Arbor lakes were made available for study through the generosity of Dr. Richard U. and Irrngard Light. The Lights also gave me the opportunity to live at Lux Arbor for the past seven years; much of the biology I have absorbed during my time here I learned through my experiences at Lux Arbor (literally a " garden of knowledge"). The lessons learned at Lux Arbor have deepened my world perspective, and I will always be grateful to Doe and Irmgard for allowing me to learn those lessons. Perhaps the most important acknowledgement is that of the friendships I have shared in here at KBS. My memories weave a rich and colorful tapestry that I will always carry with me. Much of the past several years has been lived out on the edge, but someone was always willing to explore with me and pull me back in when I ventured a bit too far. My thanks go out to those friends, namely: Mike Brown, Brian Kennedy, Kellie Ellis, Chuck Bellanger, Maile Keagle, Michelle Keagle, Jen Klug, Lars Hedin, John Ferguson, Sandy Halstead, Carolyn Miller, Joe vonFisher, Doug Jakubiak, Jackie Smith, Denise Thiede, Mark McPeek, Jackie Brown, Kathy Weist, Mathew Leibold, Kevin Geedey, Art Weist, Mark Olson, Pete Smith, Judy Teeter, Casey Huckins, Jill Fisher, Donna Fitzstephens, Lisa Huberty, Dan Detweiller, Dawn Jenkins-Klus, Paco Moore, Bryan Foster, Jean Tsao, Jeff Birdsley, Chris Rodgers, Michelle Potkin, Pete Stahl, Michel Cavigelli, Martha Tomecek, Jessica Rettig, Jenny Molloy, Tim Laatsch, Liz Smiley, Pam Woodruff, Brian Black, Rob Lu-Olendorf, Lisa Horth, Joe Picciano, Alice Gillespie, Olivia Damon, Kevin Kosola, to all of these and to many others, my gratitude for the affection and the good times and the adventures in the world we shared, that I will never forget, and that can never be lost. This work was funded by NSF Dissertation Improvement Grant BSR-9101389 to A. M. Turner, NSF Grant BSR-9207892 to G.G. Mittelbach, and the KBS Research Training Group funded by NSF grant DIR-9113598. TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES ............................. ix LIST OF FIGURES ............................. x CHAPTER 1 FRESHWATER SNAILS ALTER HABITAT USE IN RESPONSE TO PREDATORS ............. 1 INTRODUCTION .......................... 2 METHODS AND RESULTS ..................... 3 Lake Patterns ......................... 3 Are Covered Habitats Safer? .................. 9 Howls Refuge Use Induced? ................. 13 Does Habitat Structure Affect the Behavioral Responses To Predation Risk? ........................ 23 DISCUSSION ............................. 29 CHAPTER 2 SHORT-TERM AND LONG-TERM RESPONSES OF CONSUMERS AND RESOURCES TO PREDATION RISK .......................... 34 INTRODUCTION .......................... 35 METHODS ............................. 38 RESULTS ............................. 41 Snail Survivorship and Growth ................ 41 Snail Habitat Use and Periphyton Standing Crop ........ 44 DISCUSSION ............................. 49 CHAPTER 3 THE EFFECTS OF PREDATION RISK ON CONSUMER HABITAT USE AND FEEDING RATES ........ 68 INTRODUCTION ......................... 69 EFFECTS OF PREDATION RISK ON RESOURCE LEVELS . . . . 70 THE DISTRIBUTION OF CONSUMERS AMONG HABITATS OF EQUAL VALUE ......................... 75 EFFECTS OF PREDATION RISK ON CONSUMER FEEDING RATES .......................... 86 SUMMARY .............................. 94 APPENDICES APPENDIX A: SNAIL FAUNA OF EIGHT LAKES WITHIN THE LUX ARBOR RESERVE, MICHIGAN STATE UNIVERSITY . . . 97 APPENDIX B: GROWTH AND STABILITY OF RESOURCE POPULATION S .......................... 98 LIST OF REFERENCES ......................... 100 LIST OF TABLES Table Page 1 Two-way AN OVA of chemical cue effects on snail refuge use over the seven-day experiment. P-values for Time and Time x Treatment effects are adjusted (Geisser-Greenhouse correction) for a heterogenous correlation structure among dates. Error1 represents the variance due to pools within treatments, and is used to test treatment effects. Error2 is the residual error, and is used to test the effects of time (Gill 1986) ......... 24 LIST OF FIGURES Figure Page 1 An illustration of the two-level ceramic substrate used to assess the tendency of snails to use open and covered habitats in natural lakes. ................................... 5 The proportion of snails occupying the covered half of artificial substrates placed into lakes lacking and lakes containing pumpkinseed sunfish. "Combined Snails" are all taxa summed together. Vertical bars represent 1 SE, N=4 lakes ................................... 8 Survivorship of Physella in experimental pools with covered habitats absent or present, and pumpkinseeds absent or present. Vertical bars represent 1 SE, N=4 pools .................................. 12 Mean proportion of the Physella population utilizing the refuge in each of four predator cue treatments. Habitat use was censused each day 1/2 hour prior to addition of cue. Vertical bars represent 1 SE, N=4 pools ........ 17 The short-term effects of daily cue addition on refuge use. Single hatched bars represent mean habitat use 1/2 hour prior to cue addition on days four, five, and six, and double hatch represents mean habitat use 1/2 hour after cue addition on days four, five, and six. Vertical bars represent 1 SE, N=4 pools .................................. 20 The pattern of habitat use over the one-week long one experiment. The filled circles represent the mean refuge use of snails in the Control and Fish treatments, while the open circles represent the mean refuge use of snails in the Crushed Snail and Crushed Snail + Fish treatments. Vertical bars represent 1 SE, N=8 pools ........................... 22 10 11 12 Crawlout frequency in experimental pools into which Physella were exposed to two levels of simulated mortality (crushed snails added, crushed snails not added) and two sorts of habitat structure (covered habitat available, covered habitat not available). Vertical bars represent 1 SE, N =16 pools .................................. 27 Change in snail dry mass over the course of the l4—day experiment at each of the four mortality levels. Each point represents the mean of four pools i 1 SE .................................. 43 The overall pattern of snail habitat use and periphyton standing crop in relation to simulated mortality. Each point represents the mean of four pools i 1 SE. Top: Open habitat use over the entire 14-day experiment. Bottom: Periphyton standing crop on tiles in the open habitat (closed circles) and on tiles in the refuge (open triangles) at the conclusion of the experiment .................................. 46 Snapshots of snail habitat use (top) and periphyton standing crop (bottom) in relation to risk of mortality early and late in the experiment. Top row: A two-day wide window of habitat use immediately prior to the first and last periphyton sampling dates (days 3 and 14), e.g., "Early" is the mean proportion of snails using the open habitat during days two and three. Refuge use is equal to l-(open habitat use). Bottom row: The ash-free dry weight of periphyton on day 3 (Early), and on day 14 (Late). Filled circles are the standing crop of periphyton in the open habitat; open triangles are the standing crop of periphyton in the refuge .................. 48 The F-ratio of linear regressions (Ho: slope=0) performed on the relationship between mortality level and habitat use (solid circles) and periphyton standing crop (open triangles) over the course of the experiment. Habitat use data are two day means. Dashed line represents F0.05,1,14=4.60. Solid line is a cubic spline interpolation .............. 51 Use of the open habitat in the 1/4, 1, and 4 snails/day mortality treatments expressed as a percentage of the open habitat use in the 0 snails/day treatment. Data are two day means over the first eight days of the experiment .................................. 55 13 14 15 The effects of food and predation risk on the habitat use of Physella. Top: Food added to both open and covered habitats. Use of open habitat is shown under two levels of predation risk (absent, present) and two levels of food (periphyton covered tiles added to both habitats). Bottom: Food added to open habitat only. Use of open habitat is shown under two levels of predation risk (absent, present) and two levels of food in the open habitat (no spinach added, spinach added). The perception of risk was manipulated by pairing each pool with an aquaria housed in an adjacent laboratory. The predation cue was then generated by feeding five Physella daily to pumpkinseed sunfish housed in aquaria, and adding one liter of water from these aquaria, along with untreated water from control aquaria, daily to predator and no-predator pools. For both experiments "Use of Open Habitat" is the mean proportion of the population in the open habitat (j; 1 SE) over the course of the experiment, N =4 ................................ 59 Hypothetical switching curves at five levels of relative risk as suggested by Gilliam and Fraser (top) and Abrahams and Dill (bottom). Curves represent the combination of resource levels in habitats one and two which are predicted to yield equal value to foragers under 5 levels of relative predation risk in the two habitats. Curve 1 represents equal risk in each habitat; curves 2 through 5 represents the effect of increasing risk in habitat two relative to habitat one ............................ 63 Hypothetical switching curves of consumers under two levels of relative risk, and the resource depletion trajectories produced by these behaviors. Switching curves are those combinations of resource levels in habitats 1 and 2 which yield habitats of equal value to consumers under a given level of relative risk. Dashed line represents equal risk in habitats 1 and 2, solid line represents habitat 2 more dangerous than habitat 1. Consumers are predicted to deplete resources from the point shown by the open circle to the point shown by the filled circle when habitat 2 becomes more dangerous than habitat 1. Top: the pattern of habitat selection behavior and resource depletion when consumers are using the ratio rule developed by Gilliam and Fraser (1988). Bottom: the pattern of habitat selection behavior and resource depletion when consumers are using the difference rule developed by Abrahams and Dill (1989) .......................... 74 16 17 18 19 Resource production and harvest functions as a function of resource standing crop. Humped curve represents the population birth rate of resources, and the function is assumed to be equal in the two habitats. The two linear functions are the rate at which consumers in each of the two habitats harvest resources, and their slope is dependent on the distribution of consumers among habitats. The intersection of the production and harvest curves gives the equilibrium resource standing crop in each habitat (Rf, Rz‘). The model (equation 6) solves for the proportion of the consumer population needed in each habitat to maintain R1' and R; at the level of difference specified by the individual decision rule. ........... 77 The proportion of a consumer population feeding in habitat 1 as a function of the level of risk in habitat 1 relative to habitat 2. Relative risk is the ratio of mortality risk in habitat one to the mortality risk in habitat two (0 = til/#2), and is assumed here to be equal to the ratio of resources in habitats 1 and 2 (R1=QR2). Equal risk corresponds to a relative risk value of 1. In this hypothetical example a = 2 and r = 3. I show the model predictions for two densities of consumers: N = 10 consumers for the "low consumption" curve, and N = 30 consumers for the "high consumption" curve ............................... 82 The proportion of a consumer population residing in habitat 1 as a function of the level of risk in habitat 1 relative to habitat 2. Relative risk is assumed here to be equal to the difference between resources in habitats 1 and 2 (R1=Q+R2). Equal risk corresponds to a relative risk value of O. In this hypothetical example a = 2 and r = 3. I show the model predictions for two levels of grazing pressure: N = 10 consumers for the "low consumption" curve, and N = 30 consumers for the "high consumption" curve ......... 85 The predicted growth dynamics of consumers as a function of the level of risk in habitat 1 relative to habitat 2. Relative risk is defined by the ratio rule (equal risk = 1). Overall growth represents the average growth of a population, or the growth of individuals when all members of a population behave alike. Also shown are habitat-specific growth rates, which are appropriate predictors of growth if individuals specialize. Parameter values are as in Figure 3, except that N=20. Values on Y-axis are consumer feeding rates (prey eaten / time) .................. 89 20 21 The predicted growth dynamics of consumers as a function of the level of risk in habitat 1 relative to habitat 2. Relative risk is defined by the difference rule (equal risk = 0). Overall growth represents the average growth of a population, or the growth of individuals when if all members of a population behave alike. Also shown are habitat-specific growth rates, which are appropriate predictors of growth if individuals specialize. Parameter values are as in Figure 4, except that N = 10. Values on Y-axis are the number of resource individuals consumed per unit time ......... 91 The predicted pattern of consumer growth as a function of relative risk when resources are near their carrying capacity (Low Consumption, N=1O consumers) and when resources are held by consumers further below their carrying capacity (High Consumption, N=30 consumers). Top: Growth of consumers using the ratio rule, parameter values as in Figure 3. Bottom: Growth of consumers using the difference rule, parameter values as in Figure 4. Y-axis is the number of resource individuals captured per unit time .................................. 93 xiv CHAPTER 1 FRESHWATER SNAILS ALTER HABITAT USE IN RESPONSE TO PREDATORS 2 INTRODUCTION Freshwater gastropods are poorly defended against shell-crushing predators relative to their marine counterparts (V ermeij and Covich 1978). While a few freshwater species possess morphological defenses such as thick shells or large body size, most taxa are small and have surprisingly thin shells (V enneij and Covich 1978). Despite such poorly developed morphological defenses, snails are among the most ubiquitous of freshwater animals, and most snails coexist successfully with abundant predators (Lodge et a1. 1987, Osenberg 1989, Brown 1991). Verrneij and Covich (1978), Bronmark (1989), and Alexander and Covich (1991) have suggested that snails have the ability to evaluate local predation risk and select safe micro—habitats. For example, it is thought that snails may use protective microhabitats such as shallow water or the underside of logs and cobbles to a greater degree in lakes with fish predators than they do in lakes without fish predators. However, while marine gastropods are known to exhibit predator avoidance behaviors (F eder 1963, 1972, Hughes 1986) there are few studies of predator avoidance by freshwater snails. Specifically, it is not known if variation in snail habitat use among lakes is related to predator abundance, or if variation in habitat use acts to minimize mortality from predators. Further, it is not known how habitat structure might affect the avoidance tactics employed by snails. 3 In this study I present data on the habitat use of snails in natural lakes and in experimental pools aimed at answering the following four questions: 1) Is there a relationship between snail habitat use in lakes and the presence/absence of fish predators? 2) Do the behavioral patterns shown by snails function to lower their mortality rates? 3) Are the behavioral patterns flexible, and if so, how is variation induced? 4) How does habitat structure affect the avoidance tactics used by snails? METHODS AND RESULTS Lake Patterns I compared snail habitat use in four lakes containing an important snail predator, the pumpkinseed sunfish (gaping gym), to snail habitat use in four lakes lacking pumpkinseeds. Pumpkinseed sunfish are morphologically specialized molluscivores (Lauder 1983), as adults feed predominately on snails (Sadzikowski and Wallace 1976, Mittelbach 1984), and can have strong impacts on snail abundance and species composition (Osenberg 1988, Osenberg et al. 1992, Bronmark et al. 1992). The tendency of snails to use open versus covered habitats was estimated by periodically censusing their position on standardized artificial substrates. Substrates were built from two pieces of unglazed ceramic tile: a square tile (15 cm on a side) was supported by four acrylic legs 2 cm tall on one half of a second rectangular tile (30 x 15 cm). Thus, the substrates were comprised of two open surfaces (the exposed tops) and two covered surfaces (the covered Figure 1. An illustration of the two-level ceramic substrate used to assess the tendency of snails to use open and covered habitats in natural lakes. 15% 4m HTEo N SM\ 6 half of the rectangular tile and the underside of the square tile) equal in area (Figure 1). The eight study lakes lie within the Lux Arbor Reserve of Michigan State University (Barry County, Michigan). They range in surface area from 0.2 to 127 ha and in maximum depth from 1.5 to 4 m. They are permanent, hardwater lakes with rich snail assemblages similar to those found in other local lakes (Appendix A). The presence or absence of pumpkinseeds and other fish species was determined by repeated seine hauls. When present in these lakes, pumpkinseeds are quite abundant, sharing dominance with bluegills (13m macrochirus) and largemouth bass (Mm §a_lrm_idr§). Three of the lakes lacking pumpkinseeds are completely fishless, and the fourth contains fathead minnows (Pimephales promelas). I placed the four substrates 1-2 m apart on each lake bottom in water 30 to 50 cm deep on May 8, 1994, and checked them every 2 days thereafter for four weeks (N=14 sample dates). I recorded the number and identity of snails occupying the Open and covered portions of each substrate, removed the snails, and returned the substrate to the lake bottom. Data from the four substrates per lake were pooled for each sample date, and the use of cover was calculated by dividing the number of snails found under cover by the total number of snails found on the substrates. The overall mean use of covered habitats in each lake was then calculated by averaging across dates. A total of 10 taxa were found on the substrates Ma gm, Amnicola m Gflaulus parvus, Laevapex fuscus, Promenetus exacuous Gyraulus deflectus Aplexa elongata, Planorbella trivolvis Physella integra, and Pseudosuccinea columella in order of frequency of occurrence), and overall snail density averaged 0.82/substrate/date (0.09 m2 Figure 2. The proportion of snails occupying the covered half of artificial substrates placed into lakes lacking and lakes containing pumpkinseed sunfish. "Combined Snails" are all taxa summed together. Vertical bars represent 1 SE, N=4 pools. Refuge Use (%) 100 01 O Lokes Locking Pumpkinseeds E§§g Lokes Contownng Punufionseeds Conflflned SnoHs PhyseHo surface area/substrate). The pattern of snail habitat use differed markedly between lakes without pumpkinseeds and lakes with pumpkinseeds (Figure 2). Considering all snail taxa summed, use of the covered portion of substrates was 18 _+_- 9% (mean i: 1 SE, N=4) in lakes lacking pumpkinseeds, but use of the covered portion increased to 77 i 9% (mean i: 1 SE, N=4) in lakes containing pumpkinseeds (Figure 2 left, F1,6=22.1, £<.01). Because individual snail taxa did not occur on the substrates at the same frecuency in lakes with and without pumpkinseeds, the observed differences in overall snail habitat use between lake types may be due to different species-specific behaviors. However, the pulmonate snail Elyse—Ila fill—TIE was found on substrates in 6 of the 8 lakes (it was rare or absent in two of the lakes lacking pumpkinseeds), and it too used the covered portion of the substrates much more often in the presence of pumpkinseeds (Figure 2 right, F1,4=18.3, 13:.01). Although M m was the most abundant snail on the substrates (it comprised on average 24% of the snails censused), removing them from the analysis does not change the overall pattern: the remaining snail species also used covered habitats more often in the presence of pumpkinseeds (76i9% versus 22: 13%, F 15:98, £=0.03). Are Covered Habitats Safer? The differential use of covered habitats by snails in lakes with and without molluscivorous fish is consistent with the notion that covered habitats serve as a refuge from fish predation. I experimentally tested this hypothesis by manipulating the presence of fish and the presence of cover in experimental pools, and monitoring the survivorship of 10 EELSQLIQ gym. I performed this experiment in 16 polyethylene wading pools, each containing 270 liters of water (200m deep x 1.3m dia.). The pools were placed in a greenhouse at the Kellogg Biological Station, filled with well water, and allowed to aerate 48 hours. A size distribution of 335111;; representative of the overall population were collected from shallow littoral areas (< 10cm deep) of Middle Crooked Lake (one of the pumpkinseed lakes used in the habitat use measurements), and 10 snails were randomly selected and stocked into each pool. Eight of the 16 pools contained a square unglazed ceramic tile 31 cm on a side supported by four acrylic legs 2.5 cm tall. These tiles were designed to mimic natural refuges from fish predators such as the underside of logs or cobbles, and were placed into the middle of the pools, where they covered 4% of the 2.15 m2 substrate available to the snails. Pumpkinseed sunfish (105 to 120 mm Standard Length) were seined from Middle Crooked Lake and a single fish was added to each of eight pools, cross-factored with the refuge treatment. I ran the experiment for three days, checking the number of survivors and censusing their habitat use every 12 hours. Pumpkinseeds readily consumed M and had a strong effect on the number of snails surviving after three days (Figure 3; fish effect: F3,12=70.7, P < 0.01). However, the effect of pumpkinseeds on P_hyse_lla survivorship depended on whether cover was present or not; P_hy_s_e;l_l_a survivorship was higher in those fish pools with cover available (Figure 3; fish x cover interaction: F3,12=10.1, _P_ < 0.01). Thus, snails in the covered habitat are safer from pumpkinseed predation than are snails in open habitats, and cover served as a partial refuge even at the high densities of pumpkinseeds used here. 11 Figure 3. Survivorship of Physella in experimental pools with covered habitats absent or present, and pumpkinseeds absent or present. Vertical bars represent 1 SE, N=4 pools. Surfivorshk>(%fl 100 50 Pumpkinseeds Present 12 Punufldnseeds Absent 13 Pumpkinseeds also had a strong effect on the tendency of snails to use the covered habitat. Among the eight pools containing a covered habitat, the mean proportion of surviving snails found in the covered habitat averaged only 8 i 1% (i 1 SE) in the absence of fish, but averaged 37 j; 6% (j; 1 SE) in the presence of fish (fish effect: F1'6=19.2, 130.01). The habitat use pattern could not have been produced by simple depletion of snails in the open habitat by pumpkinseeds without any behavioral response by snails: the mean number of snails using the refuge over the course of the experiment was higher in the fish than in the fishless treatments (1.85 _-i_-_ 0.46 versus 0.75 i 0.17 snails (mean i 1 SE, N=4), F1.6=8.14, E = .03), despite lower overall numbers. Thus, the shift into the safer habitat in the presence of predatory fish appears to be an adaptive and flexible behavior. How is Refuge Use Induced? If M do facultativly alter their behaviors in response to the presence of predators, they must first acquire information regarding the local risk of predation. The mechanism, however, by which they might gather such information is unknown. Snails locate food patches by chemoreception (Townsend 1973, Croll 1983, Sterry et al. 1983, Bronmark 1985), and chemical cues released when predators feed on conspecifics induce changes in the life-history characteristics of m mgata (Crowl and Covich 1990). It is not known, however, if m gyrjna use chemoreception to assess predation risk. Accordingly, I conducted a second experiment designed to answer two questions: 1) Does Ma respond behaviorally to chemical cues associated with the predation process? and 2) What is the source of a cue (or cues) which may induce refuge use by Physella? 14 I tested the hypothesis that 3.115% can evaluate local predation risk and select safe habitats when exposed to chemical cues associated with the predation process by presenting various chemical stimuli to snails in wading pools and monitoring their habitat use. In mid-July, 16 pools (described above) were placed outdoors at the pond lab facility of the Kellogg Biological Station in a 4x4 grid, filled with well water, and allowed to aerate for 24 hours before snails were stocked. Each pool contained a covered habitat (hereafter the "refuge") as described above. Physelg were collected from Middle Crooked Lake, and 10 individuals were added to each pool. The following day I conducted a census of habitat use, and then imposed the experimental treatments by adding water from cue-generating aquaria assigned to one of four treatments. I imposed treatments by matching each pool with one of 16 cue-generating aquaria housed inside an adjacent laboratory. Each 36 liter aquarium was first washed with a dilute phosphoric acid solution, rinsed, filled with well water, and gently aerated with an airstone. One of four treatments was then imposed on each aquarium. "Control" aquaria received no additional treatment. "Crushed Snail" aquaria received five crushed snails daily from the same source population and of the same size as the experimental snails (snails crushed manually). The "Fish" aquaria each housed one pumpkinseed sunfish between 112 and 117 mm standard length, which were fed a single earthworm (Lumbriscus sg) daily, and the "F ish + Crushed Snail" aquaria contained a pumpkinseed sunfish which was fed five snails daily in addition to an earthworm. Each day I transferred one liter of water from each aquarium to its paired wading pool and replaced it with 15 aerated tap water. I crushed snails and fed fish one hour before the water was removed from the aquaria and added to the pools. To control for any spatial effects and insure adequate randomization, I assigned pools to treatments in a Latin-square manner, with each treatment represented just once in any row or column of pools. The experiment ran for 7 days after the treatments were first imposed on 17 July. Snail habitat use was censused in the mid-afternoon of each day prior to the addition of the aquarium water. I recorded the number of snails under the refuge, the number in the open, and the number within 1 cm of the water's surface. For each pool I estimated the probability that an individual snail would use the refuge by dividing the number of snails observed under the tile by the total number censused. In addition, on days four, five, and six I censused snail habitat use one hour following the addition of water from the cue- generating aquaria. The pattern of habitat use in the four treatments was compared to the pre-cue addition pattern with Multivariate Analysis of Variance to see if the response was time dependent. Refuge use data were analyzed using two-way repeated-measurements analysis of variance (RM-AN OVA) with crushed snail presence/absence as one factor and fish presence/absence as the other. Time (N =7 observations) was a repeated factor. Note that for the main effects and their interaction (Crushed Snails and Fish) RM-AN OVA is equivalent to taking a mean of all 7 dates for each pool, with each pool thus generating one observation, and then using two-way ANOVA to test for treatment effects. All 16 Figure 4. Mean proportion of the Physella population utilizing the refuge in each of four predator cue treatments. Habitat use was censused each day 1/2 hour prior to addition of cue. Vertical bars represent 1 SE, N=4 pools. 17 0.4 — (1) (f) 3 (I) g» .2 <1) 01 T 0.2 P 0.0 Crushed Snails: Absent Present Fish: Absent Present Absent Present 18 analyses were done using the Statistical Analysis System (SAS Institute Inc. 1991). m responded strongly to crushed conspecifics, doubling their refuge use in the crushed snail and fish + crushed snail treatments over the control and fish only treatments. Mega showed no response to the presence of fish alone (Figure 4, Table 1). These data confirm that P_hy&ll_a habitat use is a flexible behavior: the degree to which an individual uses the refuge depends on the level of predation risk perceived by that individual. The habitat use of P_hys_ella was completely contingent on the presence of crushed snails, and they were oblivious to the presence of fish (Figure 4, Table 1). This suggests that Physella respond behaviorally to a chemical cue released by injury to conspecifics, and that this one does not require modification by pumpkinseeds in order to invoke a behavioral response. Each habitat use census was conducted 23 hours after cues were last added, yet the effects were still strong. Were the short-term effects any stronger? Evening censuses conducted one hour following cue addition on days 4, 5, and 6 also found strong effects of crushed snails, but no fish effects (two-way ANOVA, Fish effect 2:027, Crushed Snail effect P=0.01, Fish x Crushed Snail effect P=0.48). However, this response was not different from the pre-cue addition response; the overall pattern of habitat use across treatments was indistinguishable one hour and 23 hours after cue addition (Figure 5, MANOVA, B > 0.10). An examination of the patterns of habitat use over time showed that refuge use was initially high, peaked on day two, and decreased significantly during the experiment (E < 0.01 for the Time effect; Fig. 6 and Table 1). However, the fish and the crushed snail 17,, 19 Figure 5. The short-term effects of daily cue addition on refuge use. Single hatched bars represent mean habitat use 1/2 hour prior to cue addition on days four, five, and six, and double hatch represents mean habitat use 1/2 hour after one addition on days four, five, and six. Vertical bars represent 1SE, N=4 pools. 20 Pre que addition § Post que oddition g: O 9. 000900060009 0 90000000900 9 new.» vvvvvvnvvvwn a.» QQQQQQQQC .99. £3.» oucuououououoooucnoucuo ooooucnouo 30090909099 900099099090 9 opovercrobmowowomouowowo» pope O 0.4 cm: mmchm Present Crushed SnoHs:Absent Present Absent Present Absent fish: 21 Figure 6. The pattern of habitat use over the one-week long cue experiment. The filled circles represent the mean refuge use of snails in the Control and Fish treatments, while the open circles represent the mean refuge use of snails in the Crushed Snail and Crushed Snail + Fish treatments. Vertical bars represent 1 SE, N=8 pools. Refuge Use 0.6 0.4 0.2 0.0 22 l l O Crushed Snoil Trt. \ O No Crushed Snoil Trt. i\§ l l —4 3 6 After Experiment Initiotion _ _.. -m LLB“! 23 treatments declined in parallel, demonstrating no treatment effects on the rate of change in refuge use (2 > 0.10 for Time*Fish and Time*Crushed Snails effects). The three way interaction of Time, Fish, and Crushed Snails was significant (P < 0.05), but is difficult to interpret. flygna may sometimes respond to predation risk by moving to the water's surface (Alexander and Covich 1991, AM. Turner primary; observation). However, I found that the number of snails utilizing the near-surface substrate (the area higher than 1 cm below the water's surface), and the proportion of snails in the open habitat using the near-surface area, did not differ among treatments (two-way ANOVA, 12 > 0.10). It is possible that movement to the surface is more likely to occur when no other refugia are available. In the next experiment I test the hypothesis that habitat structure affects the predator avoidance tactics employed by Physella. Does Habitat Structure Affect the Behavioral Response to Mortality? I tested the hypothesis that the frequency with which Ma uses the near-surface habitat depends on both habitat structure and predation risk by manipulating the presence of a covered habitat and the perception of mortality risk. This experiment was conducted in 16 wading pools (270 liters each) in the KBS greenhouse. Pools were filled with well water, allowed to aerate 24 hours, and then 10 EELS/91141 gym; collected from Lower Crooked Lake (adjacent to Middle Crooked Lake, where snails for the previous experiments were collected), were stocked into each pool. Half the pools received a covered habitat (a square tile 31 cm on a side, supported by four legs 2.5 cm tall). 24 Table 1. Two-way ANOVA of chemical cue effects on snail refuge use over the seven-day experiment. P-values for Time and Time x Treatment effects are adjusted (Geisser- Greenhouse correction) for a heterogenous correlation structure among dates. Error, represents the variance due to pools within treatments, and is used to test treatment effects. Error2 is the residual error, and is used to test the effects of time (Gill 1986). Source of Variation ss df ms F P Fish 0.0002 1 0.0002 0.02 0.90 Crushed Snails 0.1687 1 0.1687 17.54 <0.01 Fish x Crushed Snails 0.0002 1 0.0002 0.02 0.89 Block 1 (Rows) 0.0266 3 0.0088 0.92 0.48 Block 2 (Columns) 0.0137 3 0.0046 0.48 0.71 Error, 0.4039 6 0.0673 Time 1.1144 6 0.1857 12.21 <0.01 Time x Fish 0.1383 6 0.0230 1.52 0.24 Time * Crushed Snails 0.1794 6 0.0299 1.97 0.15 Time * Fish * Crushed Snails 0.4027 6 0.0671 4.41 0.02 Time "‘ Block 1 0.4505 18 0.0250 1.65 0.17 Time * Block 2 0.4346 18 0.0241 1.59 0.19 Error2 0.5477 36 0.0152 25 Cross-factored with the refuge treatment was a second treatment in which perceived mortality was manipulated. I simulated mortality on snails in the high mortality treatment by adding two crushed conspecifics each day. The snails were placed into the appropriate pools, and immediately crushed by hand. The preceding experiment demonstrates 11131me behavioral response to manually crushed snails is identical to the response to snails consumed by fish. The crushed snails were of the same size, and from the same source population, as the experimental snails. This approach is advantageous to placing a natural predator into the experimental pools because any behavioral shifts observed are not confounded with changes in snail density. Also, by crushing newly introduced snails, I avoid changing the level of past experience of the experimental snails, as would be the case if I crushed experimental snails and replaced them with conspecifics. Snail habitat use was censused in the mid-afternoon of each day, immediately before the daily mortality treatments were imposed. I recorded the number of snails under the ceramic tile, the number in the open, and the number that were at or above the waters surface. The experiment ran for five days. In addition to the daily mid-afternoon censuses, on days 3, 4, and 5, I censused snail habitat use two hours after sunset. Snails in the eight pools containing a refuge showed the same response to mortality as found in the previous experiments: 111% refuge use was higher in the presence of mortality risk than in its absence (68i5%- vs. 18i7%, F ,’6=25.4, £<0.01). The experiment as a whole revealed that use of the near-surface habitat was higher when snails were 26 Figure 7. Crawlout frequency in experimental pools into which Physella were exposed to two levels of simulated mortality (crushed snails added, crushed snails not added) and two sorts of habitat structure (covered habitat available, covered habitat not available). Vertical bars represent 1 SE, N=16 pools. Use of Near Surface Habitat (%) 27 60 gag; Refuge Absent EEEE Refuge Present 30 0 Crushed SnaHs Crushed SnaHs Absent Present +__ -.__ ,1 Figure 7. Crawlout frequ two levels of simulated sorts of habitat structi Vertical bars represrr 28 exposed to mortality risk (mortality effect F,,,2=20.5, £50.01; Figure 7). Most importantly, the response to risk clearly depended on whether a covered habitat was available or not: the risk of mortality increased near-surface habitat use much more in the absence of a covered habitat than in the presence of a covered habitat (habitat structure x mortality interaction, F = 350.01; Figure 7). The habitat use of mm; at night contrasts sharply with its daytime habitat use. Elyse—Ila abandoned both the covered and near surface habitats at night, and were found almost exclusively on the open substrates of the wading pools, even when exposed to simulated mortality. Averaging across the three nights of observations, there were no significant differences among treatments in use of covered habitats or use of near surface habitats (£90.10 for all treatment effects). Averaging across treatments, use of the covered habitats at night was only 3%, and no snails were found using the near-surface habitat. Note that this pattern can not be caused by a decay of the cue over time; the diurnal habitat use data were collected each day before the cue was added. The data show that snails moved back into covered or near-surface habitats after occupying open substrates at night. 29 DISCUSSION Behavioral flexibility may allow prey to coexist with predators while minimizing the costs of predator avoidance. In this study I have linked flexible behaviors with their survivorship functions, and shown that inter-population variation in behavior is related to the presence of predators. Payse_lla are able to evaluate local predation risk via chemical cues and facultatively adjust their habitat use by moving under covered habitats when they perceive greater risk of predation. This habitat shift appears to decrease their vulnerability to predators: covered habitats are a refuge from pumpkinseed predation. Finally, variation among lakes in snail habitat use is related to the presence of fish predators: Physella, as well as the snail community as a whole, select covered habitats more often in lakes containing pumpkinseeds than in lakes without molluscivorous fish. Like most animals, snails face a wide array of potential predators (e. g. fishes, leeches, crayfish, insects; Brown 1991), and in selecting habitats they must evaluate the level of risk posed by each predator. Predator avoidance behaviors often have significant costs associated with them (Werner et al. 1983, Skelly 1992), and obviously being preyed upon is costly. Foragers can minimize these costs by avoiding false alarms and changing their behavior only when predators pose a substantial threat (Phillips 1978). One way to accurately assess predation risk is to respond behaviorally only when predators are actively feeding on conspecifics, but this requires specific information about the diet of predators. Chemoreception potentially offers a low risk means by which prey can monitor a predator's diet. While evaluation of predation risk by chemoreception may be widespread 30 in aquatic systems (e.g. fishes, Weldon 1990, Keefe 1993, Mathis and Smith 1993; amphibians, Petranka et al. 1987, Wilson and Lefcort 1993; insects, Peckarsky 1980, Soluk and Collins 1988, Kohler and McPeek 1989, Tjossem 1990; cladocerans, Dodson 1988, Leibold 1990), few studies are adequately designed to identify the source of the chemical cue to which the prey are responding. As a result, the degree of specificity regarding the cues which induce behavioral changes is poorly understood (Wilson and Lefcort 1993). The experiments described above demonstrate that Mach—a alter their habitat use in response to a chemical cue emanating from injured conspecifics, but not in response to the mere presence of a predator. The predators of freshwater gastropods can be divided into two general classes: large predators that usually crush snails (e. g. fish, ducks, crayfish, turtles) and smaller predators that invade the shell (e.g. leeches, hemipterans, coleopterans). The behavioral responses that snails showed in the present study (moving under a refuge) are most effective against large-bodied predators that cannot follow them into confined areas. Because large predators usually crush snails, moving into a confined space is an appropriate response to cues released by a crushing action. In contrast, snails show no pre-contact response to leeches, a small predator that invades the shell (Townsend and McCarthy 1980, Bronmark and Malmqvist 1986). Instead, snails showed an escape response (shell twisting and detachment from the substrate) only after being attacked by leeches. Therefore, it appears that the behavioral response used to avoid or escape predators is linked to the type of cue used to detect each sort of predator. 31 The use of open habitats by walla in lakes lacking pumpkinseeds and at night support the notion that their behavior is adapted to minimize mortality from pumpkinseeds. Pumpkinseeds are visual predators and are not active foragers at night. Alexander and Covich (1991), however, found that in a lake dominated by crayfish predators, a nocturnal predator that relies on tactile and chemical cues to locate prey, snails showed strongest the strongest avoidance behaviors (movement to the water's surface) at night. Crayfish are very rare, or possibly absent, from my study lakes (A. M. Turner, personal observation E. Smiley, personal observation). Predator mediated behaviors have the potential to significantly alter species interactions in communities containing mobile animals (Werner 1992). Predator avoidance may levy non-lethal costs on prey; however, few studies have been able to quantify these non-lethal consequences. This stems, in part, from the difficulty of manipulating prey behavior and the methodological problem of disentangling the numerical and behavioral effects of predators on prey. However, flexible behaviors such as the habitat shifts documented in this study present valuable opportunities to measure the consequences of predator avoidance. Because Lhyarila respond to chemical cues, I can experimentally present Ease—11a, with the perception of risk and measure the various extended effects of any behavioral responses without any confounding effects of the direct mortality imposed by predators. Taxa such as M that respond to mortality on conspecifics, regardless of the source, are excellent model systems because the investigator can experimentally impose mortality risk in a highly controlled manner. 32 Although Physalla lowers its rate of mortality from fish predators by moving into safer habitats when confronted by risk, this response does not necessarily diminish the strength of food-web interactions involving predators. Because M can control resource levels within habitats (Lowe and Hunter 1988), and fish predators cause P_hy£lla to move among habitats, it seems likely that predator-induced habitat shifts by snails can have important consequences for the snail's periphyton resources. Predation risk may increase periphyton abundance in risky habitats by causing vulnerable species to move into safer habitats. A number of studies have documented that the effects of adding or removing a top predator in a food chain can be transmitted to lower trophic levels (e.g. Paine 1966), but the relative importance of numerical and behavioral effects is usually not known. For example, Bronmark et a1. (1992) showed that pumpkinseed sunfish have a positive effect on snail resources (periphyton). These effects were assumed to be numerical in nature: pumpkinseed sunfish reduced snail population sizes, thereby reducing snail grazing pressure and allowing periphyton to increase in abundance. However, the behavioral responses of snails to pumpkinseeds documented here suggests an alternative interpretation of these observations. If fish predators cause snails to move to the safety of covered habitats, then periphyton resources in more open and dangerous habitats would be expected to increase in abundance without any change in overall snail density, i.e. the indirect effects of snail predators on snail resources may be mediated though changes in snail habitat use. 33 I tested this prediction by presenting snails with the perception of mortality by adding crushed snails to experimental pools and monitoring periphyton resources grown on ceramic tiles in open habitats. I found that even though snail density was held constant, the periphyton resources increased in the open areas (a positive indirect effect) when perceived snail mortality is increased (Chapter 2). Thus, it is likely that the positive effect of snail predators on snail resources is due at least in part to the adaptive behaviors of the snails themselves. CHAPTER 2 SHORT-TERM AND LONG-TERM RESPONSES OF CONSUMERS AND RESOURCES TO PREDATION RISK 34 35 INTRODUCTION Animals often respond to increases in predation risk by moving from dangerous habitats into safer habitats (reviews in Kerfoot and Sih 1987, Dill 1987, Lima and Dill 1990). Ecological theory suggests that if animals regulate resource levels within habitats, then predator-induced habitat shifts will result in higher resource levels in more dangerous habitats relative to safer ones (Covich 1976, Abrams 1984, Kotler 1984, Gilliam and Fraser 1988, Werner 1992). Such behaviorally mediated effects of predators on resource levels may be widespread and common (e.g. Power 1984a, Power 1987, Power et al. 1985, Turner and Mittelbach 1990), yet there are still few empirical studies which allow us to gauge the necessity of incorporating habitat selection behavior 'into models of trophic interactions. Predictions regarding the short-term effects of predation risk on the habitat use of foragers are clear: if habitats are otherwise of equal value, foragers should respond to increased risk by moving into safer habitats. Resources in safe habitats will then experience higher mortality rates than resources in dangerous habitats, and resources will be depleted more rapidly in safe habitats than in dangerous ones. Over time, however, resource levels among habitats may diverge such that foragers expect equal fitness returns in high food/high risk habitats and low food/low risk habitats. Several studies have "titrated" food and predation risk, and they show that foragers will often accept a higher level of risk in return for a higher foraging rate (Gilliam and Fraser 1987, Abrahams and Dill 1989, Nonacs and Dill 1990, Todd and Cowie 1990, Kotler et al. 1991). What then, 36 will be the long-term equilibrium distribution of foragers among habitats that offer similar fitnesses, but positively covary in food and risk? Clearly, the prediction that foragers should respond to increased levels of predation risk by moving into safer habitats only works in the short-term. At present it is difficult to predict how foragers will distribute themselves among habitats that differ in both foraging rate and predation risk, but are of equal value. Experimental studies provide little insight here: there are no experiments which manipulate predation risk on foragers dynamically interacting with their own resources in two or more habitats, and record forager habitat use over time. A primary goal of this study is to follow the response of both forager habitat use and resource dynamics over time, contrasting the short-term and long-term responses of foragers and resources to manipulations of habitat-specific predation risk. Many species can detect the presence of predators and facultatively adjust their behaviors, but few studies have determined if animals can quantitatively adjust their behavior in relation to the level of risk. That is, do animals employ a fixed response to a threshold level of risk, or is their behavior modulated in relation to the increase in risk? A continuous response to increasing risk would suggest that animals are balancing the increase in risk against other, conflicting opportunities. There are potentially far-reaching implications of such a balancing act. For example, these incremental effects could potentially be transmitted to the resources, structuring them such that resource standing crop is proportional to predation risk. 37 Empirical demonstrations of behavioral indirect effects may be rare because manipulations of predator density usually result in numerical as well as behavioral effects on prey. The behavioral response is thus confounded with a numerical response, making it difficult to tease apart the relative roles of the two in affecting resources. A possible solution to this problem is to simulate mortality, thereby altering perceived mortality without actually changing prey density. Models of predators may offer a viable approach (e. g. Milinski and Heller 1978). However, the level of risk perceived by prey is not known, and prey may habituate to the model if it is not associated with mortality. A second approach is to impose known levels of mortality on experimental animals, replacing the animals killed with conspecifics so as to hold density constant (e.g. Werner et al. 1983). I use a variant on the second approach here to explore the consequences of predator avoidance by the common freshwater snail flagella m. I have shown in Chapter 1 that M gym is usually found under cover in lakes containing the molluscivorous pumpkinseed sunfish (Lappm_is_ gibbosus ), whereas in lakes lacking pumpkinseeds, P_hysalla is usually found on exposed substrates. Experiments showed that Ma offered covered substrates had lower mortality from pumpkinseeds than M offered only exposed substrates, and that the presence of pumpkinseeds cause M to move into safer (covered) habitats. I experimentally investigated the source of the cue which triggers the habitat shift, and I found that M responds to crushed conspecifics in the environment, regardless of whether the snails were crushed by a natural predator (a pumpkinseed sunfish) or by the experimenter (Chapter 1). 38 Here I present the results of an experiment in which I stocked Ma into pools with two habitats: a large open area and a smaller, covered area. A ceramic tile supporting a crop of periphyton was placed in each habitat. I then imposed four levels of simulated mortality and monitored both the habitat use of the snails and the response of their periphyton resources over time. Because PM are known to alter their habitat use in response to risk of mortality (Chapter 1), and they are known to depress resource levels in habitats in which they graze (Lowe and Hunter 1988), I hypothesized that increasing the level of mortality perceived by the snails would initially cause them to leave the open (dangerous) habitat, allowing periphyton to increase in abundance. Further, if the magnitude of the snail habitat shift depends on the level of risk, then the periphyton abundances should reflect the level of risk. Finally, I predicted that both snail habitat use and periphyton standing crop would change through time, and therefore I measured snail distributions and periphyton standing crop repeatedly over the course of the experiment. By these methods I was able to examine the behavioral indirect effects of snail predators on the snail resources without the confounding influence of numerical changes. METHODS I performed this experiment in 16 circular polyethylene wading pools, each containing 270 liters of water (20cm deep x 1.3m dia.). The pools were placed outdoors at the Kellogg Biological Station in a 4x4 grid and filled with unchlorinated well water (alkalinity equivalent to CaCO3 concentration > 100 mg/l). Each pool contained a 31 x 39 31cm unglazed ceramic tile raised off the bottom by four legs 5.5 cm tall. These tiles were designed to mimic natural refuges from predators, and were placed in the middle of the pools, where they covered 4% of the 2.15 m2 of substrate available to the snails. Thus, each pool was comprised of two habitats: a small refuge and a large open area. Twelve adult M gm were stocked into each pool on 27 July (mean shell length = 9.9 mm, mean dry mass (excluding shell material) = 10.06 mg). The snails were collected from dense aggregations (>100/m2) in shallow littoral areas (<10 cm deep) of nearby Middle Crooked Lake, and were selected to be similar in size (coefficient of variation in length = 5%). I simulated four levels of mortality on snails in the experimental pools by adding four doses of crushed conspecifics: 0, 0.25, 1, or 4 snails daily. The appropriate number of snails were placed into each pool and immediately crushed by hand. The crushed snails were of the same size and from the same population as the resident snails. I employed this design, rather than imposing mortality on a given number of snails in each pool and then replacing them with similar-sized conspecifics, because this design maintained a constant level of experience amongst the snails in a pool. The 0.25/day mortality treatment was accomplished by adding a single crushed snail on day one, and every four days thereafter. The mortality treatments were imposed daily in midafternoon. I evaluated the effect of variation in snail habitat use on resources by measuring periphyton standing crop on a pair of ceramic tiles in each pool: one under the refuge and one in the open. Small tiles (15.2 cm on a side) were first incubated for ten days in an 40 outdoor recirculating artificial stream system. Periphyton quickly grew on tiles in the stream, reaching a mean standing crop of 3.75:1.55 mg/cm2 (mean ash free dry weight i 1 SE, N=4 samples) after ten days. Two randomly selected tiles were transferred to each experimental pool (one in each habitat) on 28 July, one day after the introduction of snails. The periphyton community on the tiles was dominated by highly edible taxa. The filamentous overstory was primarily comprised of Oedogonium spp; common prostrate forms were the chlorophytes Ankistrodesmus Chlamydomonas, Scenedesmus, and Cosmarium and the diatom Navicula. I estimated periphyton standing crop on each tile over the course of the experiment by measuring Ash Free Dry Weight (AFDW) two, four, and fourteen days after the tiles were introduced to the pools. Periphyton was scraped from a strip 8.75 mm wide and running the width of the tile (13.3 cmz), rinsed onto a Whatman GF/F glass fiber filter, dried at 60° for 24 hours, weighed, ashed at 500° and reweighed. Snail habitat use was censused in the mid-afternoon of each day, immediately before the daily mortality treatments were imposed. I recorded the number of snails under the refuge, the number in the open, and the number feeding on periphyton on the small ceramic tiles in each habitat. For each pool I estimated the probability that an individual snail would use the open habitat (hereafter "open habitat use") by dividing the number of snails observed in the open by the total number censused. At the end of 14 days I collected, counted, and measured the shell length of the surviving snails. Lengths were converted into dry masses, excluding shell material, using the following length-weight 41 relation: M Dry Mass (mg) = 0.014*(Shell Length (mm))**2.86 (r2=0.86, £50.01, N =191). I analyzed the effect of simulated mortality on snail habitat use and periphyton abundance using linear regression analyses (N =16 points for all analyses). I applied a log (x+0.1) transformation to the four levels of mortality (0, 0.25, 1, and 4 snails/pool/day) in order to linearize the relationships and to equalize the influence of all four levels. Visual inspection of the residuals confirmed that there was no strong non-linearity. Regression analysis, as employed here, tests the hypothesis that there is a monotonic relationship between mortality rate and the dependent variable under consideration (slope equal to zero was used as the null hypothesis of no treatment effect). For clarity of presentation, figures show mortality as categorical data. RESULTS Snail Survivorship and Growth Snail survivorship averaged 91%, and was not related to simulated mortality rate (2 > .10). Because there were no treatment effects on snail densities, I was able to examine the consequences of variation in snail habitat use (i.e. behavioral indirect effects) without any confounding differences in snail density. Predation risk had a negative effect on the growth of Mia (Figure 8). Growth of m declined linearly as a function of mortality rate (_12 < 0.01). There were no effects of mortality rate on the within-pool variance in snail size at the end of the experiment (l_’ > 0.10). 42 Figure 8. Change in snail dry mass over the course of the 14-day experiment at each of the four mortality levels. Each point represents the mean of four pools i 1 SE. Snail Growth (mg) 2.5 2.0 1.0 43 l l l j 0 0.25 1.0 4.0 Daily Mortality 44 Snail Habitat Use and Periphyton Standing Crop How does predation risk affect m habitat use, and what effect does variation in M habitat use have on periphyton abundances? When averaged over the entire experiment, increasing the risk of mortality had a negative effect on use of the open habitat by Ma and a positive effect on periphyton standing crop in the open habitat (Figure 9, open habitat use P < 0.05, periphyton standing crop P < 0.01). Periphyton standing crop in the refuge was much lower than in the open, and there was no effect of mortality on periphyton in the refuge (Figure 9, B > 0.10) Snail habitat use did not show a threshold response to increasing predation. Instead, each successively higher level of mortality resulted in a correspondingly lower use of the open habitat (Figure 9). Periphyton standing crop, in turn, reflected this pattern of behavior by showing a gradual positive response to increasing mortality (Figure 9). While the overall effects of increasing mortality was to decrease open habitat use and increase periphyton standing crop, the strength of these responses changed over the course of the experiment. PM in high mortality treatments initially avoided open habitats, but they became indifferent to mortality by the end of the experiment (Figure 10, early mortality effect _13 < 0.01, late mortality effect P > 0.10). Conversely, periphyton standing crop in the open habitat initially showed no treatment effects, but showed a positive relationship with mortality risk at the end of the experiment (Figure 10, early mortality effect 2 > 0.10, late mortality effect P < 0.01). Periphyton standing crop in the refuge did not differ among treatments either early or late (Figure 10, B > 0.10 early and late), ‘11 45 Figure 9. The overall pattern of snail habitat use and periphyton standing crop in relation to simulated mortality. Each point represents the mean of four pools i 1 SE. Top: Open habitat use over the entire 14-day experiment. Bottom: Periphyton standing crop on tiles in the open habitat (closed circles) and on tiles in the refuge (open triangles) at the conclusion of the experiment. 0.7 0.6 Open Habitat Use 0.5 . 2 Periphyton AFDW (mg/cm ) 46 _ 0.25 1.0 4.0 § 9 . =0pen Habitat V =Covered Habitat -r- — V @ V V i l l l o 0.25 1.0 4.0 Daily Mortality 47 Figure 10. Snapshots of snail habitat use (top) and periphyton standing crop (bottom) in relation to risk of mortality early and late in the experiment. Top row: A two-day wide window of habitat use immediately prior to the first and last periphyton sampling dates (days 3 and 14), e.g., "Early" is the mean proportion of snails using the open habitat during days two and three. Refuge use is equal to 1-(open habitat use). Bottom row: The ash-free dry weight of periphyton on day 3 (Early), and on day 14 (Late). Filled circles are the standing crop of periphyton in the open habitat; open triangles are the standing crop of periphyton in the refuge. Open Habitat Use 2 AFDW (mg/cm) 0.8 0.6 0.4 0.2 Earhl 48 0.25 1.0 4.0 i I 0.25 1.0 Daily Mortality 4.0 0.8 0.6 0.4 0.2 Late oi 0.25 1.0 Daily Mortality 49 was low by the first sampling date (day 3) and remained at that low level through the experiment (Figure 10). I examined more closely the temporal pattern of habitat use and periphyton responses to mortality risk by plotting the responses over time. I used the F-Ratio of regression analyses (Ho: slope=0) performed on the relationship between mortality and snapshots of habitat use (two-day means, N=7) and open-water periphyton (N =3 sample dates) as an index of the strength of response (Cohen 1988). This analysis clearly shows that while both habitat use and periphyton were related to predation risk, the strength of the responses changed over time (Figure 11). At no point during the experiment did both habitat use and periphyton show simultaneous responses to predation risk. Instead, the responses mirrored each other, and snail habitat use and periphyton standing crop responded at different time scales to the risk of mortality. DISCUSSION This study demonstrates that @6313 change their habitat use when confronted with the risk of predation, and this change in the habitat use of P_the_lla can affect the abundances of their periphyton resources. Further, there is a strong spatial correspondence between the habitat use of Ma and the standing crop of periphyton. The habitats that were used least by M contained the highest levels of periphyton. However, the correspondence of habitat use and resource levels did not exist at any one point in time: behavioral effects were strongest early in the experiment, while the periphyton abundance effects were strongest late in the experiment. 50 Figure 11. The F-ratio of linear regressions (Ho: slope=0) performed on the relationship between mortality level and habitat use (solid circles) and periphyton standing crop (open triangles) over the course of the experiment. Habitat use data are two day means. Dashed line represents F0,05,,,,4=4.60. Solid line is a cubic spline interpolation. 51 12h e F—Ratio 0 5 10 15 Days Following Experiment Initiation 52 While the temporal segregation of behavioral and periphyton abundance effects may seem at first to be counterintuitive, it is predicted by current theory (Gilliam and Fraser 1988, Chapter 3). Consider a forager population with the following characteristics: 1) foragers regulate resource levels within habitats, 2) individuals assess habitat-specific feeding rates and mortality risks, and 3) foragers select the habitat which offers the best combination of foraging returns and mortality risk (the best value). If foragers perceive equal levels of risk in two habitats, their expected distribution should conform to an ideal free distribution (individual feeding rates in each habitat are equal, Fretwell and Lucas 1970, Fretwell 1972). If, however, one habitat becomes more dangerous (higher predation risk), then there will be a period of transitory dynamics, eventually leading to new equilibrium resource levels, and a new distribution of consumers among habitats. The second habitat (the "refuge") will initially offer foraging returns equal to the open habitat, and a lower risk of predation, so during the transition the straightforward prediction is that foragers will move into and spend all of their time in the refuge. If foragers regulate resource levels (assumption 1 above), the high grazing intensity in the refuge will eventually cause resources to be depleted to a lower level than resources in the ungrazed, dangerous habitat. At some point in time resources in the dangerous habitat may become sufficiently abundant (relative to resources in the refuge) to offset the higher risk of mortality, and some of the foragers will choose to move back out into the dangerous habitats, trading a higher risk of predation for higher foraging rates (assumption 3). Thus, given the conditions above, theory predicts that short term behavioral responses to 53 predation risk will be stronger than long-term behavioral responses. Conversely, any indirect effects of risk on resources will not be immediately manifested, but are predicted to develop during the transition as resources are differentially depleted. Resource levels in dangerous and safe habitats should then be maintained at some constant level of difference by foragers, reflecting the relative risk of the two habitats. I have shown here that the temporal patterns of Mia behavior and the temporal patterns of abundance of their periphyton resources are consistent with these predictions. The divergence in resource levels between dangerous and safe habitats occurs through the differential grazing and growth of periphyton in risky and safe habitats. Because the length of time needed for resources to diverge sufficiently to offset the higher level of risk in the dangerous habitat depends on the extent to which resources in dangerous habitats exceed levels in safe habitats, the length of the transition period (the period during which animals use only the refuge) should be related to the magnitude of danger. I found that M increased their use of the dangerous habitat over time in all treatments, but shifted to using the dangerous habitat faster at lower levels of risk (Figure 12). The overall pattern of M habitat use and periphyton abundance suggests that the snails depleted resources in the two habitats to the point where the dangerous and safe habitats were of equal value in all treatments, although it took longer for the habitats to equalize at the highest level of risk. The "depletion and tradeoff" scenario proposed here to explain the changing pattern of snail habitat use over time is not the only mechanism which could produce the 54 Figure 12. Use of the open habitat in the 1/4, 1, and 4 snails/day mortality treatments expressed as a percentage of the open habitat use in the 0 snails/day treatment. Data are two day means over the first eight days of the experiment. 55 1‘3 // 5 100 O I c (1) O. O ‘5 <1) 80 — (f) D / "O I <1) .E‘ E . O Level of Mortality 2 so ~ 2 I 0.25/day B A 1/day (I) O 4/day l l i 2 6 8 Days Since Experiment Initiation 56 pattern observed. One alternative explanation is that snails initially responded to the crushed snail cue, but then habituated to the cue after several days, and perceived a lower level of mortality. While it is difficult to know with certainty the motivations of snails for behaving as they do, there are data supporting the assumptions underlying the depletion and tradeoff mechanism. First, several studies have shown that freshwater snails, including Ma, control the abundance of their periphyton resources (Doremus and Harman 1977, Lowe and Hunter 1988, Bronmark 1989, Osenberg 1989, Barnese and Lowe 1990). In this experiment I found that M spent 45% of their time grazing on the two tile substrates, even though the two tiles comprised just 2% of the available substrate. Such an intense concentration of snail foraging activity on the tiles lends support to the notion that PpLsila interacted strongly with their periphyton resources. Second, in two separate experiments I found that M can assess habitat-specific predation risk and foraging rate, and that they will trade safety for food. Both experiments were conducted in the same pools, in the same manner, and using the snails from the same source population, as the experiment above. Habitat use analyses were done on mean habitat use over the course of the experiment (N=8 days for experiment 1, N =9 days for experiment two). In the first experiment I manipulated overall food level (tiles in open and refuge depleted by grazing prior to experiment versus tiles in open and refuge undepleted by grazing) and predation risk (two levels: 0 crushed snails/day and 3 crushed snails/day) . I found that Ma habitat use was affected by food level (F 1’,2=5.2, £<.05) and mortality level (F ,,,2=22.2, £5.01). Physella used the open habitat less in response to both 57 higher food levels and higher predation risk (Figure 13, top), demonstrating that they assess and consider both food and predators when selecting habitats. I tested the hypothesis that Mela will trade increased risk of predation for higher foraging rates by conducting a second experiment in which I manipulated food level only in the open habitat (spinach added, no spinach added) and predation risk (5 snails crushed/day versus 0 snails crushed/day). I found a strong main effect of predators on Physell_a habitat use (F 113:1 17, _E<.01), but the response to predators depended on food level (F,,6=5.6, 2:05). In the high risk treatment M ventured into the dangerous habitat most often at high food levels (Figure 13, bottom), demonstrating that they will trade safety for food. Thus, while I can not prove that the change in M habitat use through time in the main experiment was due to habituation to the mortality cue, data available from other experiments show that snails have the ability to assess food rewards and predation risk and that they will accept higher risk in return for more food. 58 Figure 13. The effects of food and predation risk on the habitat use of flryaalla Top: Food added to both open and covered habitats. Use of open habitat is shown under two levels of predation risk (absent, present) and two levels of food (periphyton covered tiles added to both habitats). Bottom: Food added to open habitat only. Use of open habitat is shown under two levels of predation risk (absent, present) and two levels of food in the open habitat (no spinach added, spinach added). The perception of risk was manipulated by pairing each pool with an aquaria housed in an adjacent laboratory. The predation cue was then generated by feeding five Bag/£112; daily to pumpkinseed sunfish housed in aquaria, and adding one liter of water from these aquaria, along with untreated water from control aquaria, daily to predator and no-predator pools. For both experiments "Use of Open Habitat" is the mean proportion of the population in the open habitat (j; 1 SE) over the course of the experiment, N=4 Open Habitat Use Open Habitat Use 0.6 59 'fifgfzgiiigigg N o F o o d Ad d e d m Food Added—Both Habitats 1.0 (18 (I6 0.4 Predation Risk Predation Risk Absent Present N 0 F0 0 d Ad d e d m Food Added—Open Habitat Predation Risk Predation Risk Absent Present 60 I have argued that the increase in periphyton standing crop in the Open habitat in high mortality treatments is due to a reduction in snail foraging. An alternative explanation is that crushing snails increased nutrient input and thereby increased periphyton abundance. Two lines of evidence argue against this. First, snail growth was negatively related to mortality level, which is consistent with the idea that differences in snail grazing is the mechanism responsible for the positive relationship between risk of mortality and periphyton abundance in the open habitat. Lower snail growth at high levels of risk reflect lower average feeding rates at high levels of risk and coincides with higher periphyton standing crop at high levels of risk. Further, snails spent a disproportionate amount of time foraging on the small periphyton-bearing tiles, suggesting that the tiles were very important food sources. Second, if nutrient input (and not snail grazing) was the predominate factor affecting periphyton standing crop, there should be a positive relationship between mortality level (number of snails crushed) and periphyton abundance in the refuge habitat. I found no relationship between mortality level and periphyton abundance in the refuge, although other factors (e.g. light levels) may have limited periphyton growth in the refuge habitat. The pattern of resource depletion in safe and dangerous habitats in this experiment offers insights into the nature of the decision rules utilized by snails when balancing predation risk and foraging rates. If a population of foragers divides its time among habitats offering equal fitness returns, but varying combinations of predation risk and foraging rates, then the amount by which food in dangerous habitats exceeds the amount 61 of food in safe habitats at one level of risk represents the energetic equivalent of that level of predation risk. Two hypotheses have been put forward regarding how animals might make this comparison. Gilliam and Fraser (1987, 1988) suggested that, given certain assumptions (most notably the presence of a foodless refuge), foragers will choose the habitat with the lowest ratio of mortality to feeding rate (i.e. minimize u/f). If foragers deplete resources and have a linear functional response, they will deplete resources such that the ratio of resources in safe and dangerous habitats matches the ratio of the predation risk (Figure 14, tOp). Abrahams and Dill ( 1989) countered with the suggestion that foragers compare the absolute difference in resource levels when selecting habitats, not the ratio of resources (Figure 14, bottom). As risk of mortality increases, the difference must be larger in order to lure foragers into the dangerous habitat (Figure 14, bottom). Both Gilliam and Fraser (GF) and Abrahams and Dill (AD) present experimental evidence to support their respective decision rules; however, the two decision rules have never been tested on a single data set. The relationship between relative resource levels and mortality risk in the experiment described here can be used to test the two decision rules. GF predicts that the ratio of resources in dangerous and safe habitats will be positively related to risk, but that the difference probably won't be (a fortuitous correlation is possible, depending on the pattern of depletion across treatments). In contrast, AD predicts that the difference will be related to risk, but it is unlikely that the ratios will be. I tested both hypotheses and found that the 62 Figure 14. Hypothetical switching curves at five levels of relative risk as suggested by Gilliam and Fraser (top) and Abrahams and Dill (bottom). Curves represent the combination of resource levels in habitats one and two which are predicted to yield equal value to foragers under 5 levels of relative predation risk in the two habitats. Curve 1 represents equal risk in each habitat; curves 2 through 5 represents the effect of increasing risk in habitat two relative to habitat one. 63 Ratio Hypothesis Resource Standing Crop — Dangerous Habitat Resource Standing Crop — Safe Habitat 2 5 4 3 Difference Hypothesis Resource Standing Crop — Safe Habitat Resource Standing Crop — Dangerous Habitat 64 difference in resource levels was related to mortality (linear regression, _13 < .01), but that the ratio of resource levels was not related to mortality (1_’ > .10), supporting the Abrahams and Dill hypothesis. The models contrasted above predict how individuals balance predation risk and foraging rate when selecting habitats, but do not address the related question of how a population of foragers will assort itself among habitats of equal value, but offering varying - _ _ l combinations of predation risk and foraging gains. In Chapter 3 I develop a three trophic : level model which uses the individual decision rules proposed by Gilliam and Fraser (1987) fifi: . and Abrahams and Dill (1989) to predict population level distributions of foragers among habitats, and their growth dynamics within habitats. The model shows that if foragers assort themselves among habitats such that they maintain high resource levels in risky habitats and low resource levels in safe habitats, thereby maintaining habitats of equal value to individual foragers, then the proportion of the grazer population in each habitat depends on how much grazing activity is needed to maintain resource levels at that level (Turner, Chapter 3). The equilibrium number of foragers in each habitat depends on habitat specific resource productivity and feeding rates. For those resource production and feeding rate functions yielding stable equilibria, more foragers are required to maintain resources at lower levels (Noy-Meir 1975, May 1977). The model thus predicts that foragers will occupy both dangerous and safe habitats, but that the density of foragers will be higher in safer habitats. Note that this result is not a direct effect of predators on forager habitat use, but instead is mediated through the indirect effects of predators on 65 resource levels and productivity. The predictions above concerning the long-term effects of predation risk on forager habitat use and resource levels can be synthesized to predict the response of forager growth to predation risk. If individual foragers within a population differ in their willingness to trade safety for food (e.g. Turner and Mittelbach 1990), then foragers will specialize. Some foragers will spend a disproportionate amount of time in risky habitats, and others will spend more time in safe habitats. Those foragers that utilize risky habitats will reap growth benefits from higher resource levels, while those that use safer habitats will suffer a growth penalty. The overall effect of increasing risk in one habitat relative to another will be to increase the variance in population growth rates (Turner and Mittelbach 1990). In this experiment I found no evidence for specialization by Mia; variance in growth was independent of risk level. What then, is the predicted effect of increasing risk on the mean growth of a forager population? In Chapter 3 I show that increasing the risk of one habitat relative to a second in a two habitat system always results in lower growth rates of foragers: the higher feeding rates foragers enjoy while foraging in the risky habitat is always more than offset by lower feeding rates in the safe habitat. The results of this experiment are consistent with this prediction: M growth at the highest level of mortality was only 59% as high as its growth at the lowest level of mortality (Figure 8). However, the magnitude of this growth penalty will be highly dependent on the overall balance of foragers and their resources (Chapter 3). In a system where resources are lightly grazed (total forager density low) 66 increasing risk will have strong negative effects on forager growth. In contrast, in overgrazed systems (total forager density high) increasing risk is predicted to have little effect on forager growth. Abrams (1992) makes similar predictions using a different modeling approach. The notion that the growth penalty associated with predation risk will vary in a predictable way with overall forager density (or inversely, underlying resource productivity) has not yet been tested, but the Ma - periphyton system used here looks promising. The strong interactions among snails, their predators, and their periphyton resources has been documented by several investigators (Osenberg 1988, 1989, Bronmark et al. 1992), but these studies focused on the numerical interactions among trophic levels. Here I have shown that snail predators can strongly interact with periphyton by changing snail behavior. Predation risk and m habitat use were negatively correlated, as was flyglla habitat use and periphyton standing crop. This pattern, typical of top-down effects, was mediated completely by behavior. These results have important implications for the interpretation of predator manipulations: behavioral mechanisms may be as important or more important than mortality mechanisms in mediating the effects of top predators on lower trophic levels. The behavioral decisions made by individual animals regarding where to forage may have repercussions which are expressed at lower trophic levels, and then feed back up the food web. Many animals alter their behavior in response to some change in the environment (e.g. predation risk or food availability). It is possible that these flexible -g. c.-. - .n 67 behavioral responses of animals to food or predators play an important role in determining the nature and strength of food web interactions (e.g. Power et al. 1985, Huntly 1987, Turner and Mittelbach 1990). "Behavioral Indirect Effects" may cascade through the food web, and have effects as strong as the numerical effects of predators on lower trophic levels (Werner 1992). Only further empirical work in which the effects of behavior are clearly isolated from numerical effects will establish whether behavioral effects are of general importance in shaping communities. CHAPTER 3 THE EFFECTS OF PREDATION RISK ON CONSUMER HABITAT USE AND FEEDING RATES 68 69 INTRODUCTION Predators have strong effects on both prey densities and prey behaviors, and these effects have been shown to cascade through food webs, indirectly affecting non-adjacent trophic levels (reviews in Kerfoot and Sih 1987, Abrams 1994). However, while there is an abundant literature on density-mediated indirect effects, there is little understanding of the potential ecological consequences of behavioral indirect effects. Consider the example of the freshwater snail flyaalla gj'ma. Where flyilla coexists with the molluscivous pumpkinseed sunfish (M M) it inhabits the underside of covered substrates, where it is relatively safe from pumpkinseed predation (Chapter 1). If invulnerable competitors are absent resources will become very abundant in open habitats, and walla will then venture forth and spend some of their time foraging in the more dangerous but profitable habitat, trading a higher risk of predation for a higher feeding rate (Chapter 2). Thus, snails in this situation divide their time between two habitats offering similar fitness returns: a safe habitat offering low foraging returns, and a more dangerous habitat offering higher foraging returns. However, there are two fundamental questions associated with this common situation that are not addressed by current ecological theory. First, what proportion of their time do we expect snails to spend in each habitat? Second, how do we expect snail feeding rates in predator-absent treatments to compare to snail feeding rates in predator-present treatments? In this chapter I will first review theoretical predictions regarding how individuals balance foraging gains and mortality risks when selecting habitats, and review how two 4 _._ 'A'- un- W I 7O decision rules have been used to predict patterns of resource depletion among habitats that differ in mortality risks. I then construct a three trophic level model aimed at predicting the distribution of consumers among habitats of equal value, but offering varying combinations of predation risk and feeding rate. The expected distribution of consumers among habitats is then synthesized with the expected resource levels among habitats to predict how consumer feeding and growth rates will respond to variation in habitat- specific predation risk. Finally, I compare these predictions to the results of experiments in which I manipulated habitat specific predation risk. EFFECTS OF PREDATION RISK ON RESOURCE LEVELS Many species of animals are capable of assessing both habitat specific predation risk and foraging rates (e. g. Chapter 2, review in Lima and Dill 1990), and a number of studies demonstrate that animals will forage in relatively dangerous habitats if food is sufficiently abundant, i.e. animals will trade a higher risk of mortality for a higher foraging rate (Gilliam and Fraser 1987, Abrahams and Dill 1989, Nonacs and Dill 1990, Todd and Cowie 1990, Kotler et al. 1991, Chapter 2). If resource levels within habitats are insensitive to variation in consumption (e. g. donor-controlled systems), then predator induced changes in habitat use will not affect resources levels. In this case, individual consumers should choose the habitat which offers the highest fitness, and the choice of habitats will not be affected by the density of consumers or the choices made by other consumers. The equilibrium distribution of consumers simply reflects the sum of their H - ‘.~--_- 71 individual decisions. However, if foragers control resource levels within habitats (Sih et al. 1985), then predator mediated changes in forager habitat use will affect resource levels. In this case, the specific patterns of resource depletion among habitats should be predictable from a knowledge of the way in which individual consumers balance energetic gains against mortality risks when choosing habitats. There is a large body of theory which specifies how individual consumers should l . balance energetic gains against mortality risks when selecting habitats (e.g., Gilliam 1982, Gilliam and Fraser 1987, Clark and Levy 1988, Mangel and Clark 1988, Abrahams and ‘1' I Dill 1989, Ludwig and Rowe 1990, Rowe and Ludwig 1991). While experimental tests of this theory are still rare, Gilliam and Fraser (1987) and Abrahams and Dill (1989) both have found that foraging fish can assess both mortality risks and energetic gains when selecting habitats, and that a higher risk of death in an area does not preclude its use. Gilliam and Fraser developed and tested a model which, given certain assumptions (most notably the presence of a foodless refuge), predicted that foragers will choose the habitat with the lowest ratio of mortality to feeding rate (i.e. minimize ,u/f). Abrahams and Dill (1989), on the other hand, suggested that foragers compare the absolute difference in resource levels when selecting habitats, not the ratio of resources. These two contrasting models can be used to illustrate how individual decision rules might affect resource depletion among habitats. Consider a two habitat system with resources (R, and R2) in each habitat, and a forager population that consumes R, and R2, but is free to choose between the two 72 habitats. The way in which consumers balance mortality risks and energetic gains when selecting habitats (i.e. their decision rules) can be represented by switching curves on a graph of R, versus R2 (Gilliam and Fraser 1988). These switching curves show the combination of resource levels in habitat one and two which yield habitats of equal value to foragers under a given level of relative risk (Figure 15). For example, if consumers perceive equal levels of risk in the two habitats, they should conform to an ideal-free I distribution among habitats (i.e. individual feeding rates in each habitat are equal; Fretwell and Lucas 1970, Sutherland and Parker 1985), and resource levels will be maintained at i equal levels in the two habitats (slope of switching curve = 1; Figure 15). If, however, habitat 2 becomes more dangerous, all consumers should initially shift to using the safer habitat (habitat 1). Over time, the higher consumption rates in habitat 1 will cause resources to be depleted there, while resources in the ungrazed habitat 2 increase in abundance (Figure 15). At some point resources in the dangerous habitat may become sufficiently abundant, relative to resources in the safe habitat, to offset the higher risk of mortality. When resources reach the new switching curve, habitats are of equal value and consumers should use both dangerous and safe habitats, depleting resources along the switching curve. Resource populations in the riskier habitat thus experience a positive indirect effect of the predator, while resources in the safer habitat experience a negative indirect effect. This positive, indirect effect of predators on resources, mediated through changes in consumer behaviors, has been shown to be an important force in natural communities (e.g. Turner and Mittelbach 1990, Chapter 2, see Abrams 1994 for a review). 73 Figure 15. Hypothetical switching curves of consumers under two levels of relative risk, and the resource depletion trajectories produced by these behaviors. Switching curves are those combinations of resource levels in habitats 1 and 2 which yield habitats of equal value to consumers under a given level of relative risk. Dashed line represents equal risk in habitats 1 and 2, solid line represents habitat 2 more dangerous than habitat 1. Consumers are predicted to deplete resources from the point shown by the open circle to the point shown by the filled circle when habitat 2 becomes more dangerous than habitat 1. Top: the pattern of habitat selection behavior and resource depletion when consumers are using the ratio rule developed by Gilliam and Fraser (1988). Bottom: the pattern of habitat selection behavior and resource depletion when consumers are using the difference rule developed by Abrahams and Dill (1989). 74 Ratio Hypothesis Resource Standing Crop — Dangerous Habitat Resource Standing Crop — Safe Habitat Difference Hypothesis Resource Standing Crop — Dangerous Habitat Resource Standing Crop — Safe Habitat 75 Behavioral rules utilized by individual foragers when selecting habitats can therefore determine the pattern of resource levels among habitats: safe habitats will have low resource levels and dangerous habitats will have high resource levels (Chapter 2). Consumer behavior effectively links together the dynamics of resources in isolated habitats: by exercising adaptive habitat selection behavior, consumers deplete resource levels to the point that habitats are of equal value. I now turn to the next question: what is l . the distribution of consumers among habitats with differing combinations of risk and resource levels, but of equal value to consumers? i “ THE DISTRIBUTION OF CONSUMERS AMONG HABITATS OF EQUAL VALUE The general approach here is to describe resource natality and mortality in two isolated habitats, and then use the decision rules reviewed above to express resource levels in one habitat as a function of resource levels in the second (Figure 16). One can then solve for the particular distribution of consumers between habitats which produces mortality functions yielding zero resource growth (Figure 16). In the following analysis I assume that: 1) resources renew logistically, 2) there is no numerical response by consumers (i.e. consumer generation times are long relative to that of resources and the frecuency of habitat switching), 3) each habitat contains a single resource population (R,), 4) consumer feeding rates are linearly related to resource density (Type I functional response), and 5) underlying resource productivity (r and K) is the same in both habitats. 76 Figure 16. Resource production and harvest functions as a function of resource standing crop. Humped curve represents the population birth rate of resources and the function is assumed to be equal in the two habitats. The two linear functions are the rate at which consumers in each of the two habitats harvest resources, and their slope is dependent on the distribution of consumers among habitats. The intersection of the production and harvest curves gives the equilibrium resource standing crop in each habitat (R,°, R;). The model (equation 6) solves for the proportion of the consumer population needed in each habitat to maintain R,’ and R2. at the level of difference specified by the individual decision rule. 77 i K \JI DH 2 < V R p O r C g m ........................ V i..R d mil. 0 LIL S e C . Fl . U n O u S " e ” DH < < O 0 USE Epic: pco cozospoca coSOmcm 78 Resource dynamics are then 7-1-RZ(1--k—)-aR2N(l-P,) (2) where P, is the proportion of the consumer population (N) feeding in habitat 1, a is the consumer attack rate on resources, r is the per capita growth rate of resources in the absence of competition, and K is the carrying capacity for the resource. Consumer behavior links resource dynamics in habitat one to resource dynamics in habitat two. Two versions of the model are developed, one using the ratio rule of Gilliam and Fraser (1988), and one using the difference rule of Abrahams and Dill (1989). By contrasting the predictions yielded by each version, I can evaluate how sensitive the model predictions are to the nature of the individual decision rule employed. In each case the variable 0 represents the energetic equivalent of a given level of relative risk. The ratio rule specifies that consumers will structure resources such that R, = 0R2, where Q =(,u,/,uz), while the difference rule specifies that resources will be structured such that R, = Q+R2. 79 Using the ratio rule, resources dynamics in habitat two can be expressed in terms of resource levels in habitat one: an, QRl 7-,QRlu- K )-.QR,N(1-P,) (3) Setting eqs. 1 and 3 equal to zero and solving for the equilibrium yields R2 rR,(l-E—)-aR2N(l-P,)=0 01' R, - (3:2) (r-aNP,) (4) K- 2R {Q :2 '-aQN(1-P,)=o (5) The proportion of the consumer population in habitat 1 (P,) can be solved for by substituting eq. 4 into eq. 5. After making this substitution, and rearranging, we find that the equilibrium distribution of the consumer population among habitats is specified by the following: P _ aN-r(l -Q) 6 ' aN(l+Q) ( ) Clearly, only certain values of a, N, and r yield a positive equilibrium for resources. Stability analysis shows that there is a single, stable non-zero equilibrium point for 80 resources in each habitat (Appendix 2): R,’ = K - aNP,K / r (7) For r < 1/2 aN, consumers always drive resources extinct, while for r > 1/2 aN there is a single stable equilibrium solution. With the presence of a stable equilibrium established, the main question of interest here can be tackled: How does the distribution of animals among habitats vary as a function of the relative predation risk of the two habitats? This model makes the simple prediction that increasing the risk of predation in one habitat relative to another will result in a lower equilibrium number of foragers in the more dangerous habitat. Figure 17 illustrates the effect of increasing relative risk in a habitat on the equilibrium number of foragers in that habitat. In addition, analysis of equation 6 shows that the results illustrated in Figure 17 holds for all parameter values: the first derivative of P, with respect to Q is negative, and the second derivative is positive. The strength of the habitat shift depends on how intensely consumers graze their resources. When resources are lightly grazed, due to low consumer population size, low consumer attack rates, or high resource growth rates, consumers feed only in the safest habitat unless relative risk is very near unity (Figure 17, "low consumption" curve). Consumers in this situation will not appreciably deplete resources in the safe habitat relative to the dangerous one, and hence will not be in a position to trade higher mortality risk for higher foraging rates. Conversely, if resources are heavily grazed, the model predicts that consumers will use both habitats over a broad range of relative risk values (Figure 17, "high consumption" curve). Thus, the sensitivity of consumers to predation 81 Figure 17. The proportion of a consumer population feeding in habitat 1 as a function of the level of risk in habitat 1 relative to habitat 2. Relative risk is the ratio of mortality risk in habitat one to the mortality risk in habitat two (0 = [ll/[12), and is assumed here to be equal to the ratio of resources in habitats 1 and 2 (R,=OR2). Equal risk corresponds to a relative risk value of 1. In this hypothetical example a = 2 and r = 3. I show the model predictions for two densities of consumers: N = 10 consumers for the "low consumption" curve, and N = 30 consumers for the "high consumption" curve. Proportion of Consumers in Habitat One (P1) 82 1.00 0.75 - < """""" Low Consumption 0.50 —< ------------------ . High Consumption E \i 0.25 — 0.00 if , 0 1 2 Relative Risk in Habitat One 83 risk will depend on the degree to which consumers hold resources below their carrying capacity. The prediction that the magnitude of the habitat shift associated with predator avoidance varies in a predictable way, depending on the relationship between consumers and their resources, is currently untested. Substituting the difference rule for the ratio rule does not substantially change the model predictions. Proceeding exactly as above, except letting R, = Q-i-R2 instead of R, = 0R2, the equilibrium distribution of consumers is given by ‘ 'Q (8) p.—.._- ' 2 KaN This version of the model also predicts that foragers will gradually shift out of the more dangerous habitat, and that the shift is more abrupt when resources are lightly grazed than when they are heavily grazed (Figure 18). The pattern shown in Figure 18 is globally true: the first derivative of P, with respect to Q is negative, and the second is 0. The overall pattern of habitat use predicted by the difference rule is remarkably similar to the pattern predicted by the ratio rule, illustrating that the predictions are not overly sensitive to the nature of the specific decision rule used. It is important to keep in mind that this model predicts the long-term equilibrium distribution of consumers among habitats. There should be a period of short-term transitory dynamics associated with changes in relative risk, illustrated in Figure 15, in which consumers will occupy the safer habitat until it depleted sufficiently to make a trade- off worthwhile (e. g. Chapter 2). The long-term solution addresses the question "how 84 Figure 18. The proportion of a consumer population residing in habitat 1 as a function of the level of risk in habitat 1 relative to habitat 2. Relative risk is assumed here to be equal to the difference between resources in habitats 1 and 2 (R,=Q+R2). Equal risk corresponds to a relative risk value of 0. In this hypothetical example a = 2 and r = 3. I show the model predictions for two levels of grazing pressure: N = 10 consumers for the "low consumption" curve, and N = 30 consumers for the "high consumption" curve. Proportion of Consumers in Habitat One (P1) 1.00 85 0.75 — 0.25 — High Consumption Low Consumption ---------->> 0.00 0 10 Relative Risk in Habitat One 20 86 many foragers are needed in each habitat to maintain resources at a level such that habitats are of equal value to consumers?" The equilibrium number of foragers in each habitat depends upon habitat-specific resource productivity and consumer feeding rates. It is well known that for the resource production and feeding rate functions yielding stable equilibria, more foragers are required to maintain resources at lower levels (Noy-Meir 1975, May 1977). This explains why consumers will move out of a habitat as it becomes more dangerous: consumers regulate resource levels such that more dangerous habitats yield higher feeding rates, and fewer consumers are required to maintain resources at higher levels. Note that this result is not a direct effect of predators on forager habitat use, but instead is mediated through the indirect effects of predators on resource levels and productivity. EFFECTS OF PREDATION RISK ON CONSUMER FEEDING RATES The effects of predation risk on individual consumer feeding and grth rates can be predicted from a knowledge of the effects of risk on consumer habitat use and the effects of risk on resource levels within habitats. Growth rates are typically positively related to feeding rates, but the nature of the function relating feeding rates to growth rates depends on a number of factors. Therefore, I take a more general approach here of modeling consumer feeding rates, and use it as a general predictor of growth. For the purposes of this analysis, I assume that the feeding rates of individual consumers residing within a particular habitat are simply (a R,), and the overall feeding rates of consumers moving 87 among habitats are 2 Ri a P,. The effect of predation risk on growth can be predicted by inserting appropriate values for R,, from the models above. Overall feeding rate of consumers is an appropriate predictor of individual growth if all individual behave alike, and each individual allocates its time among habitats as predicted by the model. However, a number of studies suggest that individuals may vary in their tendency to use risky habitats (e. g. Turner and Mittelbach 1990). In this situation, some consumers may specialize in high risk, high growth habitats, while others specialize in safe, low growth habitats. The habitat specific feeding rates are the appropriate predictors of the growth rates of habitat specialists. Using the ratio version of the model, I find that any deviation from equal risk results in reduced growth by the entire consumer population (Figure 19). However, if foragers are habitat specialists those foragers residing in risky habitats will experience a positive effect of risk on growth, while those in safe habitats will experience a negative effect of risk on growth, leading to the prediction that increasing the predation risk of one habitat relative to another will increase the variance in individual growth rates. This prediction is consistent with the pattern found by Turner and Mittelbach (1990). The difference version of the model makes growth predictions that are similar to the ratio version (Figure 20). The extent to which consumers depressed resources below their carrying capacity was found to be an important factor influencing consumer habitat use, and it also has a strong effect on the growth predictions. Both versions of the model predict that the growth penalty associated with increasing the risk in one habitat relative to another will be 88 Figure 19. The predicted growth dynamics of consumers as a function of the level of risk in habitat 1 relative to habitat 2. Relative risk is defined by the ratio rule (equal risk = 1). Overall growth represents the average growth of a population, or the growth of individuals when all members of a population behave alike. Also shown are habitat- specific growth rates, which are appropriate predictors of growth if individuals specialize. Parameter values are as in Figure 3, except that N=20. Values on Y-axis are consumer feeding rates (prey eaten / time). Consumer Growth 89 5.00 Overall Growth 2.50 — I r Growth in Habitat Two . Growth in Habitat One aoo , , Relative Risk in Habitat One 90 Figure 20. The predicted growth dynamics of consumers as a function of the level of risk in habitat 1 relative to habitat 2. Relative risk is defined by the difference rule (equal risk = 0). Overall growth represents the average growth of a population, or the growth of individuals when if all members of a population behave alike. Also shown are habitat- specific growth rates, which are appropriate predictors of growth if individuals specialize. Parameter values are as in Figure 4, except that N = 10. Values on Y-axis are the number of resource individuals consumed per unit time. sit Consumer Growth 6.00 91 4.00 ~ Growth in Habitat One Overall Growth Growth in Habitat Two 2.00 Relative Risk in Habitat One 3“] 92 Figure 21. The predicted pattern of consumer growth as a function of relative risk when resources are near their carrying capacity (Low Consumption, N=10 consumers) and when resources are held by consumers further below their carrying capacity (High Consumption, N=30 consumers). Top: Growth of consumers using the ratio rule, parameter values as in Figure 3. Bottom: Growth of consumers using the difference rule, parameter values as in Figure 4. Y-axis is the number of resource individuals captured per unit time. Consumer Growth Consumer Growth 93 4.00 - 2.00 e Low Consumption High Consumption 0.00 .I-fl 4.00 H c a , . 4 u . o Low Consumption High Consumption 2.00 Relative Risk in Habitat One 94 stronger when resources are lightly grazed (Figure 21). When resources are heavily grazed, the increase in resource standing crop associated with increased risk will stimulate resource production, and tend to ameliorate the growth penalty. This is another simple, powerful, and untested prediction: increasing the relative risk of a habitat should depress consumer grth rates, but the magnitude of the growth depression depends on how intensely consumers graze their resources. SUMMARY Perhaps the most important behavioral choice a mobile animal faces is where to forage. Early studies used long-term average rate maximization approaches to relate individual habitat choice to single resources such as mates (Orians 1969, Parker 1978), and food (Fretwell 1972, Chamov 1976, Milinski 1979, Whitham 1980, Harper 1982, Pulliam and Caraco 1984). More recent work has shown that animals are often confronted with conflicting demands, and that they may consider several factors when selecting habitats (Mittelbach 1981, Sih 1982, Werner et al. 1983, Kotler 1984, Power 1984a; review in Lima and Dill 1990). Several studies have now shown that animals will move into more dangerous habitats in return for a higher feeding rate, thereby balancing food intake against the risk of mortality (Gilliam and Fraser 1987, Abrahams and Dill 1989, Nonacs and Dill 1990, Todd and Cowie 1990, Kotler et al. 1991). Further, it is well known that resource levels in a habitat often varies inversely with consumer foraging intensity (Sih et al. 1985, Kerfoot and Sih 1987). Therefore, the level of predation risk 95 may influence food levels in a habitat through the effects of predators on consumer habitat selection behavior. The balancing acts animals perform may have major community-wide repercussions, but the importance of these extended effects are little explored. The model developed here predicts the long-term distribution and performance of consumers selecting among habitats that differ in potential feeding rates and predation risks. The model is built by incorporating a fitness currency based on feeding rates and mortality rates into an ideal-free distribution framework (Fretwell and Lucas 1970, Fretwell 1972). Assumptions of the model include 1) a linear functional response of consumers to resource density, but no numerical response by consumers, 2) that consumers trade-off mortality from predators for higher feeding rates according to some measurable rule, and 3) logistic renewal of resources. The distribution of consumers among habitats is found to depend on 1) relative risk of mortality, 2) potential resource productivity, and 3) consumer foraging intensity. Specific predictions are 1) as the relative danger of a habitat increases, consumers will gradually shift out of that habitat, 2) the habitat shift is much more abrupt when resources are lightly grazed, 3) there is always a growth penalty associated with increasing relative risk, and 4) the magnitude of the growth penalty depends on the relationship between consumers and their resources. These are questions of fundamental importance. Predicting the effects of predator and nutrient manipulations in natural communities is a major goal of community ecology, yet surprisingly little attention has been paid to linking the developing theory of habitat selection to the dynamics of resources within habitats. This theoretical framework is an 96 attempt to forge a link between the behavior of habitat selection and it's consequences for community dynamics. APPENDIX A SNAIL FAUNA OF EIGHT LAKES WITHIN THE LUX ARBOR RESERVE, MICHIGAN STATE UNIVERSITY 97 APPENDIX A Snail fauna of eight lakes within the Lux Arbor Reserve, Michigan State University. X denotes taxa present in a lake, 0 denotes taxa not found in a lake. Lake coding scheme is that devised by Elizabeth Smiley (Personal Communication). Lake LCL MCL LA02 LA08 LA09 LAl6 LA18 LA26 Area (ha) 127 78 0.2 0.3 0.4 0.1 1.0 4.0 Fish Y Ammcgle m Laevapex _fu_sc1§ Pseudosuccinea columella Fossaria humilis Physella gyrina Physella integra Aplexa elongata Planorbella trivolvis Planorbella campanulata Promenetus exacuous X X X X 0 O X X X X X 0 O O O X O X X X C O 2 O O O X X 0 O O O O O 2 X X O X 0 O X 0 O X X ~< Gyraulus deflectus O X 0 O O O X X X 0 O X ~< >< O X O X 0 O X 0 O X 0 O O O O O O O X 0 O O O 2 O X X 0 O O O X 0 O X X < O O O O Gyraulus parvus * LA08 contains fathead minnows (Pimephales promelas), but no other species of fish. All other lakes marked Y contain a typical warmwater fish assemblage (largemouth bass (Micropterus salmoides), bluegills (Lemmis macrochirus), and pumpkinseeds (L. gibbosus)). APPENDIX B GROWTH AND STABILITY OF RESOURCE POPULATIONS 98 APPENDIX B In this appendix I show through analytical methods that there is a single stable equilibrium density of resources within each patch. Noy-Meir (1975) makes similar arguments using graphical methods. The birth and death rate of resources are assumed to depend on resource density in the following manner. Per-capita birth rates decline in a linear fashion with increasing resource density, and per-capita death rates are assumed to be independent of resource density (Figure 22). The per-capita growth of resources in patch, is given by dR' (1 R') NP (A 1) =r -— -a . R,dr K ' Any positive equilibria (R') must satisfy R! r (1 -?)=3N P, (11.2) Rearranging, we find that there is a single equilibrium at R,'= K-aNP,K/r (A.3) What values of a, N, and P, result in a positive solution? R,‘ is positive when the following requirement is met: r > a N P, (A.4) Therefore, r < 1/2 aN will never yield a positive equilibrium; consumers always drive resources extinct. 99 A positive equilibrium will be locally stable if :1 (1R, — — < 0 (A5) dR, R,dt at equilibrium. Substituting equation A.1 into A.5 yields 4 R1 r JR, [r(l- K)-3NP,) -K (A.6) Since r and K are by definition positive, -(r/K) is always negative, giving the result that R,’ will always be a locally stable equilibrium point, and that resources will always increase from low density (given R,‘ > 0). LIST OF REFERENCES LIST OF REFERENCES Abrahams, M.V., and L.M. Dill. 1989. A determination of the energetic equivalence of the risk of predation. Ecology 70:999-1007. Abrams, PA. 1984. Foraging time optimization and interactions in f00d webs. American Naturalist 124:80-96. Abrams, PA. 1992. Why don't predators have positive effects on prey populations? Evolutionary Ecology 6:449-457. Abrams, RA. 1994. Indirect and direct interactions in food webs; definitions and consequences. unpublished manuscript Alexander, J .E., and AP. Covich. 1991. Predator avoidance by the freshwater snail Physella virgata in response to the crayfish Procambarus sirnulans. Oecologia 87:435- 442. Bronmark, C. 1985. Interactions between macrophytes, epiphytes, and herbivores: an experimental approach. Oikos 45:26-30. Bronmark, C. 1989. Interactions between epiphytes, macrophytes, and freshwater snails: a review. Journal of Molluscan Studies 55:299-311. Bronmark, C., and B. Malmqvist. 1986. Interactions between the leech Glossiphonia complanata and its gastropod prey. Oecologia 69:268-276. Bronmark, C., S.P. Klosiewski, and RA. Stein. 1992. Indirect effects of predation in a freshwater, benthic food chain. Ecology 73:1662-1674. Brown, K. 1991. Mollusca: gastropods. m J. Thorp and A. Covich, editors: Ecology and classification of North American freshwater invertebrates. Academic Press, Inc. San Diego, California. Chamov, EL. 1976. Optimal foraging, the marginal value theorem. Theor. Pop. Biol. 9:129—136. 100 101 Clark, C.W., and DA. Levy. 1988. Diel vertical migrations by juvenile sockeye salmon and the antipredation window. Am. Nat. 110:141-151. Cohen, J. 1988. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, New Jersey. Covich, AP. 1976. Analyzing shapes of foraging areas: some ecological and economic theories. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics 7:235-237. Croll, RF. 1983. Chemoreception. Biological Reviews 58:293-319. Crowl, TA, and AP. Covich. 1990. Predator-induced life-history shifts in a freshwater snail. Science 247:949-951. Dill, L.M. 1987. Animal decision making and its ecological consecuences: the future of aquatic ecology and behaviour. Canadian Journal of Zoology 65:803-811. Dodson, S. 1988. The ecological role of chemical stimuli for the zooplankton: Predator avoidance behavior in Daphnia. Limnology and Oceanography 33:1431-1439. Doremus, C.M., and W.N. Harman. 1977. The effects of grazing by physid and planorbid freshwater snails on periphyton. Nautilus 91:92-96. Feder, HM. 1963. Gastropod defensive responses and their effectiveness in reducing predation by starfishes. Ecology 44:505-512. Feder, HM. 1972. Escape responses in marine invertebrates. Scientific American 227 292-100 Fretwell, SD. 1972. Populations in a seasonal environment. Princeton University Press, Princeton, NJ. Fretwell, SD. and H.L. Lucas, Jr. 1970. On territorial behavior and other factors influencing habitat distribution in birds. 1. Theoretical development. Acta Biotheor. 19:16-36. Gilliam, J .F. 1982. Habitat use and competitive bottlenecks in size-structured fish populations. Dissertation. Michigan State University, East Lansing, Michigan. 102 Gilliam, J .F., and DP. Fraser. 1987. Habitat selection under predation hazard: test of a model with foraging minnows. Ecology 68:1856-1862. Gilliam, J .F., and DP. Fraser. 1988. Resource depletion and habitat segregation by competitors under predation hazard. Pages 173-184 E B. Ebenman and L. Persson, editors. Size-Structured Populations: ecology and evolution. Springer-Verlag, New York, New York. Harper, D.G.C. 1982. Competitive foraging in mallards:'Ideal-free' ducks. Animal Behaviour: 30:575-584. Hughes, RN. 1986. A functional biology of marine gastropods. The Johns Hopkins University Press, Baltimore, Maryland. Huntly, NJ. 1987. Influence of refuging consumers (Pikas: Ochotona princeps) on subalpine meadow vegetation. Ecology 68:274-283. Keefe, M. 1992. Chemically mediated avoidance behavior in wild brook trout, Salvelinus fontinalis: the response to familiar and unfamiliar predaceous fishes and the influence of fish diet. Canadian Journal of Zoology 70:288-292. Kerfoot, W.C., and A. Sih. 1987. Predation: direct and indirect impacts on aquatic communities. University Press of New England, Hanover, NH. Kohler, S.L., and MA. McPeek. 1989. Predation risk and the foraging behavior of competing stream insects. Ecology 70:1811-1825. Kotler, BR 1984. Risk of predation and the structure of desert rodent communities. Ecology 65:689-701. Kotler, B.P., J .S. Brown, and O. Hasson. 1991. Factors affecting gerbil foraging behavior and rates of owl predation. Ecology 72:2249-2260. Lauder, G.V. 1983. Functional and morphological bases of trophic specialization in sunfishes (T eleostei, Centrarchidae). Journal of Morphology 178:1-21. Leibold, MA. 1990. Resources and predators can affect the vertical destributions of zooplankton. Limnology and Oceanography 35:938-944. Lima, S.L. and L.M. Dill. 1990. Behavioral decisions made under the risk of predation: a review and prospectus. Canadian Journal of Zoology 68:619-640. 103 Lodge, D.M., K.M. Brown, S.P. Klosiewski, R.A. Stein, A.P. Covich, B.K. Leathers, and C. Bronmark. 1987. Distribution of freshwater snails: spatial scale and the relative importance of physicochemical and biotic factors. American Malacological Bulletin 5:73-84. Lowe R.L., and RD. Hunter. 1988. Effect of grazing by Physella integra on periphyton community structure. Journal of the North American Benthological Society 7:29-36. Ludwig, D., and L. Rowe. 1990. Life-history strategies for energy gain and predator avoidance under time constraints. Am. Nat. 135:686-707. Mathis, A., and J .F. Smith. 1993. Fathead minnows, Pirnephales promelas, learn to recognize northern pike, Esox lucius, as predators on the basis of chemical stimuli from minnows in the pike's diet. Animal Behavior 46:645-656. Mangel, M., and CW. Clark. 1988. Dynamic Modeling in Behavioral Ecology. Princeton University Press, Princeton, New Jersey. May, R.M. 1977. Thresholds and breakpoints in ecosystems with a multiplicity of stable states.Nature 269:471-477. Milinski, M. 1979. An evolutionary stable feeding strategy in sticklebacks. Z. Tierpsychol., 51:36-40. Milinski, M, and R. Heller. 1978. Influence of a predator an the optimal foraging behavior of stickleback (Gasterosteus aculeatus L.). Nature 275:642-644. Mittelbach, G.G. 1981. Foraging efficiency and body size: a study of optimal diet and habitat use by bluegills. Ecology 62:1370-1386. Mittelbach, G.G. 1984. Predation and resource partitioning in two sunfishes (Centrarchidae). Ecology 65:499-513. Nonacs, P., and L.M. Dill. 1990. Mortality risk vs. food quality trade-offs in a common currency: ant patch preferences. Ecology 71:1886-1892. Noy-Meir, I. 1975. Stability of grazing systems: an application of predator-prey graphs. Journal of Ecology 63:459-481. 59‘ “Mi?!“i'ml" 104 Orians, G.H. 1969. On the evolution of mating systems in birds and mammals. Am. Nat. 103:589-603. Osenberg, CW. 1988. Body size and the interaction of fish predation and food limitation in a freshwater snail community. Dissertation. Michigan State University, East Lansing, Michigan, USA. Osenberg, CW. 1989. Resource limitation, competition, and the influence of life history in a freshwater snail community. Oecologia 79:512-519. Osenberg, C.W., G.G. Mittelbach, and PC. Wainwright. 1992. Two-stage life histories in fish: the interaction between juvenile competition and adult performance. Ecology 73:255-267. Paine, RT. 1966. Food web complexity and species diversity. American Naturalist 100:65-75. Parker, G.A. 1978. Searching for mates. In J .R. Krebs and NB. Davies, eds. Behavioral Ecology: An Evolutionary Approach, Blackwell Scientific, Oxford. Peckarsky, BL. 1980. Predator-prey interactions between stoneflies and mayflies: Behavioral observations. Ecology 61:932-943. Petranka, J.W., L.B. Kats, and A. Sih. 1987. Predator-prey interactions among fish and larval amphibians: use of chemical cues to detect predatory fish. Animal Behavior 35: 420-425. Phillips, D.W. 1978. Chemical mediation of invertebrate defensive behaviors and the ability to distinguish between foraging and inactive predators. Marine Biology 49:237-243. Power, M.E. 1984a. Habitat quality and the distribution of algae grazing catfish in a Panamanian stream. J. Anim. Ecol. 53:357-374. Power, M.E. 1984b. Depth distributions of armored catfish: predator-induced resource avoidance? Ecology 65:523-528. Power, ME. 1987 . Predator avoidance by grazing fishes in temperate and tropical streams: importance of stream depth and prey size. Pages 333-352 i_n_ W.C. Kerfoot and A. Sih, editors. Predation: direct and indirect impacts on aquatic communities. New England University Press, Hanover, New Hampshire, USA. 105 Power, M.E., W.J. Matthews, and A.J. Stewart. 1985. Grazing minnows,piscivorous bass and stream algae: dynamics of a strong interaction. Ecology 66:1448-1456. Pulliam, H.R., and T. Caraco. 1984. Living in groups: Is there an optimal group size? m J .R. Krebs and NB. Davies, eds. Behavioral Ecology: An Evolutionary Approach, Blackwell Scientific, Oxford. Rowe, L. and Lugwig, D. 1991. Time constraints and variation in size and timing of metamorphosis in complex life cycles. Ecology 72:413-427. Sadzikowski, M.R., and DC. Wallace. 1976. A comparison of food habits of size classes of three sunfishes (Lepomis macrochirus Rafinescue, a gibbosus Linneaus, and a cyanellus Rafinescue). American Midland Naturalist 95:220-225. Sih, A. 1982. Optimal patch use: variation in selective pressure for efficient foraging. American Naturalist 120:666-685. Sih, A., P.H. Crowley, M.A. McPeek, J .W. Petranka, and K. Strohmeier. 1985 . Predation, competition, and prey communities: a review of field experiments. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics 16:269-311. Skelly, OK. 1992. Field evidence for a cost of behavioral anti-predator response in a larval amphibian. Ecology 73:704-708. Soluk, DA, and NC Collins. 1988. Balancing risks? Responses and nonresponses of mayfly larvae to fish and stonefly predators. Oecologia 77:370-374. Sterry, P.R., J.D. Thomas, and R.L. Patience. 1983. Behavioral responses of Biomphalaria glabrata (Say) to chemical factors from aquatic macrophytes including decaying Lemna paucicostata (Hegelm ex Engelm). Freshwater Biology 13:465-476. Sutherland, W.J., and G.A. Parker. 1985 . Distribution of unequal competitors ip Behavioural Ecology: Ecological Consecuences of Adaptive Behavior. Editors Sibly, R.M., and RH. Smith. Blackwell Press, Oxford. Tjossem, SJ. 1990. Effects of fish chemical cues on vertical migration behavior of Chaoborus. Limnology and Oceanography 35:1456-1468. Todd, I.A., and R.J. Cowie. 1990. Measuring the risk of predation in an energy currency: field experiments with foraging tits, Parus caeruleus. Animal Behavior 40:112-117. 106 Townsend, C. R. 1973. The role of the osphradium in chemoreception by the snail Biomphalaria glabrata (Say). Animal Behavior 21:549-556. Townsend, C. R. 1975. Strategic aspects of time allocation in the ecology of a freshwater pulmonate snail. Oecologia 19: 105-115. Townsend, C.R., and T.K. McCarthy. 1980. On the defense strategy of Physa fontinalis (L.), a freshwater pulmonate snail. Oecologia 46:75-79. Turner, A.M, and G.G. Mittelbach. 1990. Predator avoidance and community structure: interactions among piscivores, planktivores, and plankton. Ecology 71:2241-2254. Verrneij, G.J., and A. P. Covich. 1978. Coevolution of freshwater gastropods and their predators. American Naturalist 112:833-843. Weldon, PJ. 1990. Responses by vertebrates to chemicals from predators. Ia Chemical signals in vertebrates. V. Edited lay D.W. Macdonald, D. Muller-Schwarz, and SE. Natynczuk. Oxford University Press, Oxford. pp. 500-521. Werner, BE. 1992. Individual behavior and higher-order species interactions. American Naturalist 140:85-832. Werner, E.E, J .F. Gilliam, D.J. Hall, and G.G. Mittelbach. 1983. An experimental test of the effects of predation risk on habitat use in fish. Ecology 64: 1540-1548. Whitham, T.G. 1980. The theory of habitat selection examined and extended using Perphigus aphids. Am. Nat. 115:449-466. Wilson, D.J., and H. Lefcort. 1993. The effect of predator diet on the alarm response of red-legged frog, Rana aurora tadpoles. Animal Behavior 46:1017-1019. _—_ . 11111111111111