\W”limlflllmlllflmWWIWWW““WWW —{ 3:33 _.05) . A Fisher z (z=1.41, p=.079) revealed that the difference between these correlations approached significance. The significant positive correlation for the hypnosis group suggests that the experimental manipulation did in fact increase suggestibility. This increase occurred not on the basis of hypnosis per se, but on the basis of hypnotic depth (i.e. hypnotic susceptibility). Hypothesis 4. Hypothesis 4 stated that the ability to experience a hypnotically induced dream is a function of the degree to which waking mentation is under the sway of primary process. A Mann-Whitney U test was used to compare Rorschach- manifested primary process of those able to experience a hypnotically induced dream with those unable to experience a hypnotically induced dream (Table 2). The outcome of primary process for this variable (hypnotic dream ability) was potentially confounded by overall hypnotic susceptibility: however, the nonsignificant correlation of hypnotic susceptibility with primary process eliminated this concern. Unexpectedly, subjects who were able to experience or report having a hypnotically induced dream had a lower percentage of primary process in their responses than those who were unable to have a hypnotically induced dream. Subjects who were able to have a dream averaged 54.6% primary process in their Rorschach responses, while those who were unable to have a dream averaged 64.3%. Those who were able to experience a 28 hypnotically induced dream also manifested significantly less Level 1 responses (i.e. direct, intense, or blatant manifestations of primary process) than those who were unable to have a hypnotically induced dream (Table 2). The former had an average of 14.0% of their responses manifesting Level 1 primary process, in contrast with the latter who had an average of 20.5% Level 1 responses. When interpreting these results, one must be mindful that the recording of the ability to experience a hypnotically induced dream depended upon the subject's ability or willingness to report the dream. Table 2 Mann-Whitney U test comparing primary*prooess on the basis of hypnotic dream ability D£2;!_QIP____!9_D£2§H_§IP 44! P Total Primary Process .546 .643 116.5 .023 Level 1 Prim. Proc. .140 .205 114.5 .020 Level 2 Prim. Proc. .472 .539 148.0 .169 Content Prim. Proc. .444 .531 113.5 .074 Formal Prim. Proc. .187 .250 131.5 .065 Entries for the dream and no dream groups are means. 29 Table 3 Correlation matrix for both groups combined HYP HYPDPP V800 RPP L1PP LZPP OONPP PORMPP HY? ---- .51.. .19 -.13 ‘.29 -.01 -.05 -.24 EYPDPP‘ ---- .10 -.23 -.40** -.02 -.02 -.51** V800 ---- -.32* -.13 -.30* -.37* -.09 RPP ---- .49** .85“ .77.. .59** L1PP ---- .15 .23 .73.. LZPP ---- .93** .22 OONPP ---- .12 PORNPP ---- tp<.05, 00p<.01 HYP=Total score on hypnotic scale (8H88:C) HYPDPP=Degree of primary process in the hypnotically induced dream V80G=Total score on virtual suggestibility scale RPP=Tota1 primary process on the Rorschach L1PP=Level 1 primary process on the Rorschach L2PP=Level 2 primary process on the Rorschach OONPP=Content primary process on the Rorschach PORMPP= Formal primary process on the Rorschach Table 4 Correlation matrix HYPDPP V800 L1PP L2PP FORNPP *p<.05, HYPDPP ttp<.01 30 for the hypnosis group V800 RPP .40. -e11 .05** -.21 -e33 L1PP -e3’ .40 L2PP -e13 .03** -e03 HYP=Total score on hypnotic scale (SHSS:C) HYPDPP=Degree of primary process in the hypnotically induced dream VBUG=Total score on virtual suggestibility scale RPP=Total primary process on the Rorschach L1PP=Level 1 primary process on the Rorschach L2PP=Level 2 primary process on the Rorschach CONPP=Content primary process on the Rorschach PORMPP= Formal primary process on the Rorschach OONPP PORKPP .09 -.33 -.07 -.49* -.10 -.50** .77** .67** .12 .73** .93** .31 ---- .22 31 Table 5 Correlation matrix for the Rorschach group HYP HYPDPP V806 RPP LlPP L2PP OONPP PORMPP HYP ---- .50** .04 -.20 -.10 -.25 -.22 -.22 HYPDPP ---- -.34 -.09 -.41 -.20 .10 -.51* V800 ---- -.35 .06 -.55* -.49* .17 RPP ---- .55* .05** .749. .49. L1PP ---- .26 .27 .72** L2PP ---- .93** .10 OONPP ---- -.02 PORKPP ---- *p<.05, **p<.01 HYP=Total score on hypnotic scale (SHSS:C) HYPDPP=Degree of primary process in the hypnotically induced dream VBUG=Total score on virtual suggestibility scale RPP=Total primary process on the Rorschach L1PP=Level 1 primary process on the Rorschach L2PP=Level 2 primary process on the Rorschach CONPP=Content primary process on the Rorschach PORMPP= Formal primary process on the Rorschach 32 Wings Tables 3, 4, and 5 show correlation matrices for all measures and their specific components. Several noteworthy correlations were obtained. Significant positive correlations were obtained for both groups between level 2 primary process (L2PP) and content primary process (CONPP) . This correlation may be due to a scoring artifact in the Holt system, as content responses usually are automatically assigned a level 2 designation unless they are extraordinarily blatant. Also, positive correlations were obtained for both groups between level 1 primary process (L1PP) and formal primary process (FORMPP). Again, these correlations may be the result of a scoring artifact; in the Holt system, many formal primary process responses (e.g. inappropriately mixing color with form) are automatically assigned a level 1 designation. High positive correlations were obtained between the overall score of Rorschach primary process (RPP) and each subcomponent of Rorschach primary process. These correlations indicate that each subscale is positively contributing to the overall primary process score, which lends construct validity to the Holt scoring system. These correlations also indicate that overall Rorschach primary process is a representative indication of all sub-categories of primary process: hence, this variable was given the most weight when interpreting the relation between Rorschach primary process and other variables. Significant positive correlations were obtained for both 33 groups between the degree of primary process in the hypnotically induced dream (HYPDPP) and the total score on the hypnotic susceptibility scale (HYP) . These correlations imply that the ability to engage in primary process during hypnosis may be a reliable indicator for hypnotic susceptibility: similarly, on Freud's account, the higher degree of primary process as hypnotic depth increases indicates that the state of hypnosis is accompanied by a regression. As Gill and Brennan (1959) state, "the ready inducibility of dreams in hypnosis is an indication of the regressive nature of the hypnotic state. " In sum, hypnotic depth seems to be marked by a regression, which is consistent with the theory put forth by Freud. An unexpected difference in sign was found for the correlation between virtual suggestibility (VSUG) and primary process in the hypnotically induced dream (HYPDPP) for the two groups: -.34 for the Rorschach group and .65 for the hypnosis group. The two correlations were significantly different (Fisher 2 = 3.29, p=.0005). The obtained correlation of .65 for the hypnosis group indicates that for subjects initially receiving hypnosis, hypnotic primary process in the induced dream was related to increased waking suggestibility. If hypnosis is in fact a regression characterized by primary process, this regression should subsequently affect suggestibility by reinforcing the subject’ s identification with the experimenter (Freud, 1921). The identification may thereby increase the willingness of the subject to affirm 34 suggestions made by the experimenter. This finding is consistent with the possibility that the heightened suggestibility is still present after the formal hypnotic termination. Moreover, this large correlational difference lends strong support to the state theory of hypnosis. Proponents of this theory such as E. Fromm contend that hypnosis is an altered state of consciousness. Fromm asserts that in hypnosis, primary process, fantasy, and ego receptivity gain greater dominance (1979). The fact that an opposite between-group difference was obtained implies a categorical difference in the state of consciousness between hypnosis and the waking state, with hypnosis being characterized by greater regression. Furthermore, the state theory is bolstered by the between group correlational difference for virtual suggestibility (VSUG) and hypnotic susceptibility (HYP): .04 for the Rorschach group and .48 for the hypnosis group. If hypnosis does accompany greater ego receptivity as From contends, then one would expect that subjects in the hypnosis group would be more receptive to suggestions made immediately after hypnosis. Also supporting the state theory of hypnosis are the negative correlations found in both groups between overall Rorschach primary process (RPP) and the hypnosis variables. While primary process during hypnosis (HYPDPP) was positively related to hypnotic depth (HYP), primary process during the waking state (RPP) was negatively related to hypnotic depth. Moreover, the correlation between primary process during the 35 dream (HYPDPP) and primary process during the waking state (RPP) was negative. These findings again imply that hypnosis is a distinct state of consciousness, as contended by Reyher, Fromm, and others. This support for the state theory of hypnosis is strengthened by the fact that the same method (i.e. the Holt primary process scoring system) was used to determine primary process in hypnosis and on the Rorschach. The average card to card manifestations of virtual suggestibility were calculated and graphed for each group (Figure 1). There were no significant between group differences for any of the cards. Response percentages were subsequently averaged for each virtual suggestion according to suggestion type (popular, unusual, or negative form quality). The popular suggestions had the highest percentage of affirmative responses (.825), followed by the unusual suggestions (.558) and the negative form quality suggestions (.347). These findings lend reliability to the virtual suggestion scale as this was the expected order of suggestion affirmation. Furthermore, there was a remarkable and uniform decrease in suggestibility after the virtual suggestion of "lettuce" was given for Card IV. Virtual suggestibility thereafter'decreased for all three suggestion types on Card‘V. Reasons for this finding will be discussed below. Gender effects were also evaluated for subjects and experimenters (Table 6). There were no significant sex of subject effects. However, there were significant experimenter effects for several of the primary process variables. 36 mecca noncomtox mmoto< >u.._n_umomm:m .mauc_> _ cs:m_m 28 5.522. 2 a a h a m .. n N _ Ti 1 .. l I u u i u a 3 95 5.2626 .G. — 28w 5:288 Eu 335; .u. w.— a w.“ «2.0 5.523. as! bananas... .823 37 Subjects having the male experimenter manifested an average of 64.6% primary process responses on the Rorschach, while subjects having the female experimenter manifested an average of 53.9% primary process responses (U=106.5, p=.01) . Likewise, subjects having the male experimenter had greater Level 1 primary process than those having the female experimenter (U=76, p=.001) . Finally, subjects having the male experimenter also had greater formal primary process than subjects having the female experimenter (U=94.5, p=.004). Table 6 Mann-Whitney U test comparing primary process on the basis of sex of experimenter MAP—LL! O E P. Total Primary Process .539 .646 106.5 .010 Level 1 Prim. Proc. .121 .221 076.0 .001 Level 2 Prim. Proo. .475 .532 160.0 .209 Content Prim. Prcc. .452 .519 151.5 .192 Formal Prim. Proc. .169 .265 094.5 .004 Entries for the male and female groups are means. The average card to card manifestations of primary process were calculated and graphed for each group (Figure 2) . Two cards elicited significant between-group differences. For Card II, the hypnosis group had an average of 43% primary process responses, while the Rorschach group had an average of ‘69% primary process responses. Similarly, for Card Ix, the hypnosis group had an average of 53% primary process responses, while the Rorschach group had 83% primary process responses. It is also noteworthy that the four largest between group differences were all color cards (Cards II, 38 VIII, IX, and X): for each of these cards, the hypnosis group consistently had less primary process than the Rorschach group. Finally, frequency distributions for the SHSS:C scores are shown in Figures 3, 4, and 5. 39 i meemu consumLOm mmOco< mmouota >tmE_ta N ot:m_m 2.0 5822. a u n a m .. a a _ b n a q ‘- Cub al- db 9 new someones: new apnoea»: er: /\ d. 330 5.55: “33 use; 2.2.... 382.. are... 3 33:33.. 40 muuomasm __< tom =o_u=n_tu._o cream a mmzm m otam_a 28m onmmzm 2 a a m u m .. a a — . a 55:3: 28m “6°35 =4 8". 8:353...— 28m onmwzm 41 cacao mwmo:a>: och tom co_u:nmtum~o etoum unmmzm : ct:m_u 28m 013:... 2 a a h u m _. a a _ . a .. 2 . _ . w.— . a 5:33.". 28m . 3 . a . m.» . ._ .520 e355: 2: a“. 8:355 28m 98% 42 esotu sumsomtoz ugh Lem co_u:n_tum_c occum uummzm m ctzmmm S 23w 0%me a a a m ._ a N _ 3 2 N .0532“. 23w 2. a w.” _. 32o 5.522. 2: 3". 5.59.56 23m 93% DISCUSSION The first hypothesis, stating that hypnotizability is a function of the degree to which waking mentation is under the sway of primary process, was not corroborated. This finding may bring increased clarity to J. Hilgard's conception of the hypnotically susceptible personality. As mentioned above, Hilgard's research led to the conclusion that imaginative involvement seems to play a role in hypnotizability: characteristics such as being deeply imaginative and absorbed in fantasy were found to be related to susceptibility. When examined in light of the results of the first hypothesis, imaginative involvement (as conceptualized by Hilgard) and primary process (as conceptualized by Freud and Holt) do not seem to be completely compatible and synonymous. There may be two possible explanations for this discordance. Firstly, the difference may be due to the fact that Hilgard's study and the present study used different methods. While Hilgard utilized the interview format, the present study utilized the Rorschach. As noted by Kazdin (1980, p. 248) , using different methods of assessment has the distinct possibility of leading to divergent results, given that "an individual’s performance on a given measure is partially a function of the precise 43 44 method in which assessment is conducted." Kazdin notes that factors such as "the contribution of method variance of different assessment devices" and "the multifaceted nature of personality and behavior” can lead to a lack of correspondence among methods of measurement . The second possible reason for the discord is that imaginative involvement and primary process simply may not connote the same underlying concept. Even though Hilgard speaks of suspension of reality testing and an engagement in vivid fantasy, these concepts are necessary but not sufficient characteristics for primary process. Primary process seems to be a more precise concept, referring to regression and ideational wish-fulfillment of aggressive and sexual strivings. Freud's concept seems more specific in that it refers to a very definite process of the dream-work rather than just fantasy in general. In sum, the results from hypothesis one suggest that imaginative involvement and primary process cannot be used interchangeably. The unexpected result for hypothesis 2 showed that subjects in the Rorschach group manifested greater primary process than the hypnosis group. Perhaps this result is due to hypnosis being a state of regression which allows for greater gratification of an unconscious fantasy. One index of this fantasy is libidinal investment (cathexis) in the hypnotist, which may thereby reduce the need for subsequent gratification in the Rorschach. If one considers hypnosis as an opportunity for libidinal cathexis or primary process 45 manifestation, then the need for primary process expression after hypnosis may be reduced. This possibility seems to fit in well with Freud’s as well as Gill and Brennan’s theory of hypnosis. In W, Freud (1900) likens self-observation to the mental process of falling asleep and hypnosis: in these states of mind, the critical faculty of the mind is relaxed, allowing involuntary ideas to emerge. This emergence, Freud states, accompanies a shift toward primary-process functioning. Freud (1921) later explained the hypnotic state and its relation to being in love in W: "Hypnosis resembles being in love in being limited to these two persons, but it is based entirely on sexual impulsions that are inhibited in their aim, and puts the object in the place of the ego ideal" (p. 96). Freud expands on this statement by comparing neurosis to hypnosis: "it [neurosis] resembles hypnosis and group formation in having the character of a regression, which is absent from being in love" (p. 97). If hypnosis has "the character of a regression," then this state should be marked by increased primary process mentation. Gill and Brenman (1959) also view hypnosis as involving regression. In elaborating on the regressed state, Gill and Brenman describe the manner in which primary process gains dominance: ”A regressive process [such as hypnosis] is one in which the balance of forces shifts so that freer and more primitive impulses come to expression, while the control system likewise becomes more primitive and relatively less stringent and 46 determining of the course of psychic life vis-a-vis the impulses" (p. 106) . Again, when hypnosis is conceptualized as involving regression, then this state of mind may provide an increased opportunity for mental drive gratification and wish fulfillment. This psychoanalytic approach has been corroborated by recent research. In a comprehensive review of studies on hypnosis and regression, Nash (1988) concluded that "there is evidence that hypnosis enables subjects to elicit more imagistic, primary process, and affect-laden material (topographic regression)" in addition to a relaxation of defenses (p. 383) . From this perspective, the hypnosis group may have had lower primary process on the Rorschach because of the regnance of primary process which immediately preceded the Rorschach: because of the regressive nature of hypnosis, subsequent manifestations of primary process may have been reduced, given that hypnosis had already provided the possibility of wish-fulfillment. Congruent with this explanation is the finding that the Rorschach group had significantly greater primary process than the hypnosis group on two of the color cards. Similarly, the four largest between group differences were all for color cards, with the uniform trend of the Rorschach group expressing greater primary process than the hypnosis group. Perhaps these findings can be understood from Shapiro's (1977) theory of the Rorschach color response. Shapiro reviews past research and theories concerning color perception, concluding that color perception and color experience is primarily a 47 passive and immediate process, requiring less complex psychological organization. Shapiro notes the "compelling, gripping quality" of the color cards, stating that there is often "an increase in the significance of color under circumstances of disorganization or primitivization of thought" (p. 262) . From Shapiro's perspective, color can provide the opportunity for the experience of primary process: given the passivity and decreased psychological organization that often accompanies color perception, the color cards of the Rorschach may have set the stage for greater primary process manifestations. Moreover, when color is conceived as being conducive to primary process expression, the findings concerning the group differences corroborate the aforementioned view that hypnosis is an opportunity for regressive or primary process mentation. In other words, the Rorschach group had greater primary process on four of the color cards because they provided the catalyst for primary process, which had already been experienced by the hypnosis group and perhaps was not as pressing. The results for the third hypothesis revealed no significant between group difference for the virtual suggestions. However, a significant positive correlation was obtained for between the virtual suggestibility scale and the hypnotic susceptibility scale in the hypnosis group, but not the Rorschach group. This finding necessitates a modification of the notion that "hypersuggestibility" persists after hypnosis has been formally terminated. As Krueger and Hull 48 contended, heightened suggestibility does seem to remain in effect after hypnosis, but the degree of this post-hypnotic suggestibility is connected to the degree of the subject’s hypnotic depth. As the results of the present study suggest, the more susceptible a subject is, the more suggestibility will be heightened during the period immediately fellowing hypnosis. Hypnotic depth, not merely the act of being hypnotized or taken through a hypnotic procedure, seems to be the factor related to post-hypnotic hypersuggestibility. Despite the analogous results in the present study and.Krueger and Hull’ 8 studies, the two sets of findings cannot be cogently compared because of the widely divergent methodology. While Krueger and Hull used eye closure as a measure for "hypersuggestibility," the present study used Rorschach suggestion affirmation to determine suggestibility. Because of this. difference, the: two studies cannot be reliably compared. Furthermore, of the five subjects that Krueger used, "all had been previously used in hypnotic experiments” (1931, p. 135). From this statement one could possibly surmise that Krueger's subjects were all capable of significant hypnotic depth and had a more established transference with the experimenter. .Again, these differences between studies preclude their comparison. Moreover, more research is needed to corroborate the possibility that post- hypnotic suggestibility increases as hypnotic susceptibility increases. The unexpected result for the fourth.hypothesis revealed 49 that subjects who were able to experience a hypnotically induced dream had lower Rorschach-manifested primary process than those unable to experience a hypnotically induced dream. Perhaps this finding can be understood when viewing both the hypnotically induced dream and the Rorschach as opportunities for primary process expression. From this perspective, those unable or unwilling to experience a dream would be expected to have greater primary process manifestations on the Rorschach. In other words, for those able to experience the dream, the motivation for Rorschach-manifested primary process expression may have been reduced because the dream had offered an opportunity for regression and mentation marked by wish- fulfillment. Likewise, for the Rorschach group, those subjects having high primary process manifestations may have experienced a reduced need or motivation to have a hypnotically induced dream, again because a chance had already arisen (in the Rorschach) for wishful mentation. In conclusion, the fact that the no dream group had higher Rorschach-manifested primary process than the dream group may highlight the possibility of viewing both the Rorschach and hypnotic dreams as opportunities for wish-fulfillment. Concerning the additional findings of the present study, an inspection of the virtual suggestions given during the Rorschach revealed a remarkable and uniform decrease in suggestibility after the virtual suggestion of "lettuce” was given for Card IV. Freud's conception of the counter-will can be used to understand this finding. Freud (1893, 1921) 50 conceptualized the counter-will as a psychic mechanism containing antithetical ideas that are opposed to conscious ideas. According to Freud, the counter-will acts as follows: ”In all renunciations and limitations imposed upon the ego a periodical infringement of the prohibition is the rule...the abrogation of the [ego] ideal would necessarily be a magnificent festival for the ego, which might then once again feel satisfied with itself" (1921, p. 81). Applying Freud's concept to the present study, if one conceives the virtual suggestions as "renunciations and limitations imposed upon the ego," then the counter-will is instrumental in abrogating the prohibitions by temporary suggestion negation. In other words, the virtual suggestion of "lettuce" may have been such an excessive request- (imposition) that the counter-will rebelled by negating the subsequent suggestions. One can also apply Brehm’s (1966) theory of psychological reactance to better understand this finding and provide a reinterpretation of Freud's. counter-will concept. IBriefly stated, Brehm contended that "when a person believes himself free to engage 'in a given behavior, he will experience psychological reactance if that freedom is eliminated or threatened with elimination. Psychological reactance is defined as a motivational state directed toward the re-establishment of the threatened or eliminated freedom, and it should manifest in increased desire to engage in the relevant behavior and actual attempts to»engage in it" (pp. 15-16). Applied.to the present study, subjects' "freedom" to perceive what they did and did 51 not see in the ink blots was constantly being challenged and compromised by the experimenter’s virtual suggestions. Perhaps subjects were willing to confirm the experimenter’s suggestions, but this willingness was limited. When subjects' realized that their freedom of individual perception was being threatened, they may have become temporarily motivated to reestablish their freedom in the form of negating the virtual suggestions. Perhaps this realization was provoked by the somewhat outrageous suggestion on Card 4 that the "blot also forms lettuce." After this suggestion was made, not only was a sharp decline in suggestibility found for the lettuce suggestion, but there was also a decline for the three suggestions following the lettuce item. At no other time was this tendency of decline found for the suggestibility scale. From Brehm’s perspective, the suggestion of seeing lettuce in Card 4 posed too much of a threat to the subjects' freedom, and was thereby followed by psychological reactance in the form of suggestion negation. Nevertheless, these post hoc conjectures must be verified by future research. One possible way to test this finding would be to utilize the lettuce suggestion as well as other suggestions that are extremely inconsistent with the perceptual possibilities of blot's form. Suggestion order should be randomized to help assure that psychological reactance is taking place. Concerning the gender difference found for the experimenters, subjects having the male experimenter expressed greater primary process on the Rorschach than subjects having 52 the female experimenter. There are at least two possible explanations for this finding. One explanation involves the divergent personality styles of the two experimenters. The male experimenter seemed comfortable, confident, and non- threatening. When observed, he executed the experimental protocol the calm, self—assured style. The female experimenter, while competent, seemed nervous, self-conscious, and displayed a lack of confidence when performing the experiment. Although these descriptions are derived from the researcher’s own impressions rather than scientific data, they may provide an explanation for the differences in primary process manifestations. Subjects having the male experimenter may have felt more relaxed, secure, and candid in expressing their perceptions of the ink blots. These feelings seem to have accompanied a relaxation of defenses and a greater engagement in primary process mentation. Subjects having the female experimenter may have been more reluctant to engage in primary process because of her nervousness. This explanation may be supplemented when considering a Freudian analysis of the results. In W, Freud (1918) investigated the dynamics of primitive societies. In this work, Freud frequently spoke of the tribal chief or ruler, who was regarded with great reverence. Freud noted that primitive societies believed the chief to possess enormous, almost super-human power. Furthermore, the chieftain rouses envy and wishful thinking over his people: "everybody would perhaps like to be king" (p. 45) , because "these savage kings are 53 endowed with a wealth of power and an ability to bestow happiness which only gods possess" (p. 59). In other words, Freud contended that the tribal kings acted as a source of gratification. These emotions, claims Freud, "arise from the infantile attitude of the child to its father" (p. 68), as omnipotence is regularly attributed to the father in the imagination of the child. But these dynamics not only occurred in primitive societies, but.may also become manifest in regressive processes (p. 24), such as hypnosis. Gill and Brenman (1959) support this connection: "one important aspect of the transference which emerges in hypnosis is the fantasy of the limitless power of the hypnotist" (p. 74). Furthermore, this position has been supported by recent research; Nash (1988) found that the perceived power of the hypnotist and a fear of negative appraisal correlate significantly with hypnotic susceptibility. Keeping these statements in mind, the fantasy of the hypnotist's omnipotence may elicit greater wish-oriented mentation (i.e. primary process). Furthermore, the gender difference found for the experimenters may be due to the fact that the male experimenter acted with more authority, command, and control, thereby eliciting these fantasies to a greater extent. And from Freud's viewpoint, the male experimenter would be more likely to resurrect infantile fantasies concerning the omnipotent father, thereby eliciting greater primary process. REFERENCES REFERENCES Binet, A., & Fere, C. (1888). W. D. Appleton and Company. Bowers, P. G. (1982). The classic suggestion effect: Relationships with scales of hypnotizability, effortless experiencing, and imagery vividness. .Inrernerienel MW , 19. 270' 279. Brehm, J. W. (1966). h o s c 0 'ca eacta c . New York: Academic Press. Brenman, M., 8 Reichard, S. (1943). Use of the Rorschach test in the prediction of hypnotizability. fielle;ie_er W19 1. 183-187. Cohen. 3.. 8 Cohen. P- (1983). Annlied_uultinle_899r99919n Hillsdale, New Jersey: Lawrence Erbaum Associates. DePaulo, B. M., Kenny, D. A., Hoover, C. W., Webb, W., 5 Oliver, P. V. (1987). Accuracy of person perception: Do people know what kinds of perceptions they convey? geernel 9: Personality ang geeiel Psychology, fig, 303- 315. Diment, A. D., Walker, W. L., 8 Hammer, A. G. (1981). Response to poetry in hypnosis and the waking state: A study of non-suggested aspects of the hypnotic state. t a ' 'cal d menta mm. 2. 19-40- Exner. J- E- (1990) - WM MW Asheville: Rorschach Workshops. Freud, 8. (1891/1966). Hypnosis (J. Strachey, Trans.). 59911991111511.9199. l. 105-144- London: Hogarth- Freud, S. (1893/1966). A case of successful treatment by hypnotism (J - Strachey. Trauma-)- 991199999_£sp_ers. 5. 33-47. New York: Basic Books. 54 55 Freud. S- (1900/1965)- W (J- Strachey, Trans). New York: Avon Books. Freud, S. (1913). .IQLQHIQDQJIQDQQ (A. A. Brill, Trans). New York: Vintage Books. Freud, S. (1921). Egg (J. Strachey, Trans.). New York: Bantam Books. Fromm, E. (1979). The nature of hypnosis and other altered states of consciousness: An ego-psychological theory. In E. Fromm 8 R. E. Shor (Ed. ), e s c . New York: Aldine Publishing Company. Fromm, E. (1984). Hypnoanalysis with particular emphasis on the borderline patient- 2929hoanslxti9_2929holesx. 61-76. Fromm, E. 8 Shor, R. E. (1979) . Underlying theoretical issues: An introduction. In E. From 8 R. E. Shor Eerepeeriyee. New York: Aldine Publishing Company. Galin, D. (1974). Implications for psychiatry of left and right cerebral specialization. Arehives of Geeerel Ministry. 11. 572-581- Gill. M- M. 8: Brenman. 14- (1959)- W t ° 8 ° e s . New York: International Universities Press, 1959. Goffman, E. (1959) . s 'o ' v Lire. Garden City, NY: Doubleday. Graham, K. R. (1977). Perceptual processes and hypnosis: Support for a cognitive-state theory based on laterality. Appeal Hey Xerk ecegemy of Science, 274-283. Graham, K. R., 8 Pernicano, K. (1979). Laterality, hypnosis, and the autokinetic effect. WW . Hypnosis. 22. 274-283- Gur, R. C. 8 Gur, R. E. (1974). Handedness, sex, and eyedness as moderating variables in the relation between hypnotic susceptibility and functional brain asymmetry. W. 9.3.. 644-650- Hilgard. E- (1965)- W192 New York: Harcourt, Brace 8 World, Inc. Hilgard. J- (1970). W. Chicagm The University of Chicago Press. 56 Holt, R. R. (1977). A method for assessing primary process manifestations and their control in Rorschach responses. In M. A. Rickers-Ovsiankina (Ed.) , MW. Huntington, NY: Robert E. Krieger Publishing Company. Hull, C. (1933/1968). 0 ' es ' . New York: Meredith Corporation. Kazdin, A. E. (1980). Peyehelegy. New York: Harper and Row. Kihlstrom, J. F. (1985). Hypnosis. u v w f £9¥9h91991..1§. 385-418. Kihlstrom, J. F. (1989). Dispositional correlates of hypnosis: A phenomenological approach. The In99rna919nal__l99rnal__9f__9l1n19al__999__912911191991 32999919. 11. 249-263. Krueger, R. G. (1931). Variation in hypersuggestibility preceding, during and following the hypnotic trance. I9urna1_9f_Abn9rmal.9nd_§99isl_£929991991. 29. 131-140- Lynn, S. J., 8 Rhue, J. W. (1986). The fantasy-prone person: Hypnosis, imagination, and creativity. gegrnel__e£ E9r99nal192.9nd_§99111_£§x9991992. 51. 404-408. MacLeod-Morgan, CL, Lack, I“ (1982). Hemispheric specificity: A physiological concomitant of hypnotizability- £929n92nx9191292. 12. 687-690. Moore, R. K. (1964). Susceptibility' to hypnosis and susceptibility to social influence. our 0 and_§99ial_2929991991. 98. 282-294- Nash, M. R. (1988L Hypnosis as a window on regression. Bull991n.91_9h9_Henn1nger_911119. 52. 383-403- Reyher, J. (1973). Can hypnotized subjects simulate waking behavior? W. 31-36. Reyher, J., Wilson, J. G., 8 Hughes, R. P. (1979). Suggestibility and type of interpersonal relationship: Special implications for the patient-practitioner relationship. J99rnal_9f.3999919h_1n_2919991119¥. 175-186. Sarbin, T. R., 8 Madow, L. W. (1942). Predicting the depth of hypnosis by means of the Rorschach test. Amerieen I9urnal_of_9r9h929¥9_1atrx.12. 268- -271. 57 Sackeim, H. A., Paulus, D., 8 Weiman, A. L. (1979). Classroom seating and hypnotic susceptibility. geernel W992. 99. 81-84. Shapiro, D. (1977) . A perceptual understanding of color response. In M. A. Rickers-Ovsiankina (Ed.), Berseheeh Beyehelegy. Huntington, NY: Robert E. Krieger Publishing Company. Sheehan, P. W. (1982L Imagery and hypnosis - Forging a link. at least in part. 13W WW. 1. 257-272- Smyth, L. D., 8 Reyher, J. (1971). Attention and suggestibility: The special implications for personality differences between high and low responsive persons. Unpublished manuscript, Michigan State University. Weitzenhoffer, A. M., 8 Hilgard, E. R. (1962). . mm s ' t . Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press. White, R. W. (1937) . Two types of hypnotic trance and their personality correlates. W. 3.. 265- 277. Wilson, 8. C., 8 Barber, T. X. (1981). Vivid fantasy and hallucinatory abilities in the life histories of excellent hypnotic subjects ("somnambules") : Preliminary report with female subjects. In E. Klinger (Ed.), V e lts ' (pp. 133-149). New York: Plenum Press. Wilson, 8. C., 8 Barber, T. X. (1983). The fantasy-prone personality: Implications for understanding imagery, hypnosis, and parapsychological phenomena. In A. A. Shaikh (Ed ). WM epplleerlen (pp. 340-390). New York: Wiley. "Iliiiiiifiiiiiliii'“