' '1", .l. . f'. . 35‘! 5... .5: fififimfiiapv it? i... .1 3th.. e ..:7 .i «I. J . I II. 2‘. i :3... [It 33:23 {LII}, I {-91.1 In 2. . 1.. S? i. n. 2303135.. i 6.13:2.-. . .5... 1.55.; 13.1.... ’I» ksxrramnfsq u? I a v3 4.5.72.1?! . I. {itzzugnifi 5.4:)»: nary) 1r: . 7 . o Ellyn... 5: $1.. :1 53>.& ‘ n» V . .2. 453.....137 , 1.....I‘talst .x:v;1i.;.\.t. . 5‘. I. i inhuman. ‘155? availz. .I..V1.b.t:1 il::ll$)‘$£l¢t!lllxlai : .) 3.1.19’ is .uv... iii: .2; : ) c... I nixxnxanstil (2:1 .- I. . 1’ an. . v . a : f... :25. I 1 $5.91... .2. :I .. it 96‘: «‘19:...iaa’kft Lat.) ‘ .SVO\§13A;I IrivAQI; .42).“. i 1.2.x; 2d; . .1333»: ?11 .UI! VAL" )0 v!!! . V 3‘»! n. .n yhnvar-S.» 2.33%.]. an... . I. It»?! i« :5. 1! (ill. Iv}!!! . 135:! I. YR). 711’!!va “pl. liliiaOPAvD sill! at: 1..» 1.3";(3 n \bs...lx7$vv VESIR ILTV LIIBRAR IES WWilliilmlilililiWWII ill ll "‘ 3 1293 014172 This is to certify that the dissertation entitled Depression and Anxiety: Distinctions and Commonalities presented by Marilyn Bleiweiss Charles has been accepted towards fulfillment I of the requirements for Doctoral degree in Clinical Psychology Date 8/3/95 I r MS U is an Affirmative Action/Equal Opportunity Institution 0-12771 Mm A_~—.__.- .. n v.— LIBRARY Mlchtgan State University PLACE II RETURN BOX to remove We checked Irom your record. To AVOID FINES return on or before dete due. DATE DUE, DATE DUE DATE DUE PRU; 92080 ‘ ‘ '1 1 l. DEPRESSION AND ANXIETY: DISTINCTIONS AND COMMONALITIES By Marilyn Bleiweiss Charles A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Psychology 1995 ABSTRACT DEPRESSION AND ANXIETY: DISTINCTIONS AND COMMONALITIES By Marilyn Bleiweiss Charles Overlap of symptoms and high comorbidity rates have made it difficult to discriminate anxiety and depression as discrete entities. Self-report instruments which broadly sampled symptoms associated with these two disorders were administered to college students in an attempt to determine whether there are distinct clusters of symptoms which can discriminate between these two constructs. Factor analyses linked somatic symptoms of motor tension and autonomic hyperarousal most specifically to anxiety, and fatigue and hopelessness most specifically to depression, supporting the recent findings by Watson and Clark (1995). Further analyses did not support models in which symptoms of anxiety and depression are viewed as largely expressions of a single factor of “neuroticism” or “negative afi‘ectivity;” the analyses pointed to clear and potentially meaningful distinctions between symptoms, in spite of their high correlations. A content analysis of widely used instruments for measuring anxiety and depression suggested that the anxiety scales under consideration are more representative of the relevant syndrome than are the depression scales. This may be due in part to the greater homogeneity in the construct of anxiety. Dedication: To my family, who have taught me to value the important things in life: my parents, who taught me to value Integrity, my sisters, who taught me of real Friendship, Bruce, who taught me of Love with no limits, Devon, who taught me the Joy of the interpersonal world, Justin, who taught me Faith in the spiritual world, and Jonathan, who taught me Courage: to believe in myself and those around me. iii ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS I acknowledge with gratitude the efi‘orts of my committee members for their assistance in completing what has been for me both an arduous and affirming process. As I moved through my internship year, and began to make the transition from “student” to “professional” in my clinical work, I was aware of parallels with the dissertation, which felt stalled and unwieldy. Much as in a fairytale, I moved toward completing this process, hitting barriers which had to be slain or hacked through in order to reach the castle; these barriers were largely my own fears of what lay beyond. It brought to mind a fairytale I learned from my mother about Diana, who was so swift that no one could catch her. She was challenged to many races, until, finally, an admirer threw golden apples in her path. As she stooped to catch them, and dragged them along in her skirts, she was slowed down. At this point in time, I don’t recall whether she won the race; I know that she finished. I have been thrown many “golden apples” in my life, and have found that the heaviest and most difiicult to shoulder often contained the most enduring and precious gifts. And so, I would like to express my deep appreciation toward the people who have helped me to shoulder my burdens, and in that process, to believe in myself. Susan Frank was the first professor at State who really invested her time and energy toward facilitating my growth. Her remarkable intellectual and clinical acuity set a iv standard which both drew and intimidated me, and ultimately encouraged me to set my own. Susan opened many doors for me both clinically and academically; she challenged my limits and helped me to find and develop my own unique resources. Most importantly, she provided a receptive and constructively critically ear, helping me to refine and structure the ideas which motivated this dissertation. Her faith and friendship have meant a great deal. Jack Hunter has devoted himself intensively and without reservation to this project. He has challenged me to clarify, refine, and articulate my thoughts and to then stand by them. His investment and enthusiasm in this project, his intense intellectual curiosity, and his passion for understanding have helped to revitalize this project and to keep me engaged even in the face of discouragement and fi'ustration. John Hurley has been a source of support and encouragement throughout my time at State. His warmth, humour, and enthusiasm initially brought me here, and he has always been ready with assistance: from theoretical issues, to editing, to moral support. His unswerving belief in me has brought me through many of the hills and valley I have encountered in this process. Ellen Strommen deserves special acknowledgement for providing the impetus to finally move this process to completion. She has helped to humanize my graduate experience by speaking for, providing a forum for, and exemplifying the values of relationship and care. Her warmth, humour, optimism, and common sense have helped to turn many apparent mountains back into hills. There are many other individuals who have supported my grth over the past six years, and have helped me to believe in, embrace, and utilize my strengths and gifts. Notable amongst these are Ed Gibeau, Ruth Rosenthal, and Gordon Williams; their unswerving faith has helped me to find my own: a tremendous gilt. Finally, I would like to acknowledge the patience, understanding, and support of my family, who have struggled through this whole process with me. I appreciate their willingness to accept all the challenges which this life has brought them: to work through them, and to grow through them. vi TABLE OF CONTENTS Page LIST OF TABLES ............................................................................................................... ix Chapter I. INTRODUCTION ......................................................................................... 1 II. THEORETICAL BACKGROUND ............................................................... 3 Symptoms .......................................................................................... 3 Diathesis of Anxiety and Depressive Disorders ................................ 6 Physiological Studies ....................................................................... 13 “Learned Helplessness” and “Serotonin Driven Depression” ......... 17 Assessment ...................................................................................... 23 Common Concomitants .................................................................. 24 Anaclitic and Introjective Depression ............................................... 28 Difi‘erences Between Anxiety Symptoms .......................................... 40 Summary ......................................................................................... 41 III. PLAN OF ANALYSIS .............................................................................. 43 IV. METHOD ................................................................................................... 46 Participants ..................................................................................... 46 Instruments ..................................................................................... 46 Procedure ...................................................................................... 53 V. RESULTS .................................................................................................... 54 Hierarchical Structure of the Data Analysis .................................... 54 Items and Symptoms ....................................................................... 62 Symptoms and Syndromes .............................................................. 76 Results Pertaining to a One Factor Model ...................................... 97 vii Chapter Page VI. DISCUSSION ........................................................................................... 105 Negative Afi‘ectivity: A General Distress Factor? ......................... 106 Confirmation and Disconfirrnation of Claims ................................ 115 Current Anxiety and Depression Scales ........................................... 120 Symptoms of Anxiety .................................................................. 122 Heterogeneity in the Construct of Depression ............................. 123 Anxiety and Depression .............................................................. 124 Conclusions as to the Measurement of Anxiety and Depression... 127 APPENDIX A ....................................................................................................... 129 APPENDD( B ...................................................................................................... 144 REFERENCES ................................................................................................... 153 viii Table 10. 11. 12. LIST OF TABLES Page Rates of Anxiety and Depressive Disorders in Children and Relatives ..................... 9 Contingency Table for the Anaclitic and Introjective Scales ................................. 32 Proposed Proportions of Anaclitic and Introjective Depression in the General Population per 100 People ...................................................................... 33 Proposed Proportions of Anaclitic and Introjective Depression in a Patient Population per 100 Depressed People ...................................................... 34 Proposed Proportions of Anaclitic and Introjective Depression in an Outpatient Population in Which There are 40% Depressives ................................. 35 The Correlation Between Anaclitic and Introjective Style Scales as Reported in Various Studies ................................................................................. 36 Meta-Analysis of Correlations Between Anaclitic and Introjective Style Scales .................................................................................................................... 37 Predicted Sums of Levels for Identification of Anaclitic and Introjective Depressives ........................................................................................................... 39 Number of Items and Reliabilities for Symptoms Retained for Further Analysis ................................................................................................................. 75 The Exploratory Factor Analysis of the Symptom Construct Correlation Matrix ................................................................................................................... 78 Provisional Clusters Suggested by Exploratory Factor Analysis ........................... 83 The Confirrnatory Factor Loadings for Construct Correlation Matrix Defined by 10 Symptom Clusters ......................................................................... 85 ix Table Page 13. The Confirmatory Factor Loadings for the Symptoms Not Used to Define Clusters in the Analysis of the Symptom Construct Correlation Matrix Defined by 10 Symptom Clusters ............................................................................ 87 14. Correlations Between Syndromes ........................................................................ 89 15. Correlations Between Need for Others and Mixed Depression, Severe Symptoms, and Alienation ...................................................................................... 9O 16. Symptom Construct Correlation Matrix (Corrected for Attenuation) ...................... 93 17. Symptom Scale Correlation Matrix (Not Corrected for Attenuation) ................... 95 18. Correlations Between Pure Anxiety, Hostility, and Pure Depression ..................... 97 19. Content Analysis of the Items Considered in the Watson and Colleagues (1995b) Study ..................................................................................................... 108 20. Number of Items from Each Cluster Represented in Anxiety and Depression Instruments ............................................................................................................ 121 21. Contingency Table Relating Pure Depression and Pure Anxiety ........................... 125 22. Correlation Matrix for all Scales Defined by Content Analysis ........................... 144 CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION Depression and anxiety are each complex syndromes, with diverse symptoms, etiological factors, and treatments. This complexity has made it difficult to understand and treat these disorders. There are two factors in particular which have made it difficult to discriminate anxiety and depressive disorders as discrete entities. These factors have complicated research on the etiology and treatment of these disorders. One factor has to do with the high correlations reported between anxiety and depression. There are several reasons for these high correlations. First, there are overlapping symptoms between the two disorders. Second, because of overlapping symptoms, there are measurement problems in distinguishing these two syndromes. There have also been arguments for common etiological factors underlying these disorders (Merikangas, 1990). High comorbidityrates have been reported for these two disorders (Kashani et. al., 1987a; Kashani et al., 1987b; Weissman et al., 1987); researchers have found that as many as 87.5% of individuals diagnosed with major depression had also had an anxiety disorder at some time (Weissman, Leckman, Merikangas, Gammon, & Prusofi‘, 1984). There is now strong evidence for a theory that anxiety has a direct effect on depression (van Praag, 1994). In addition, some researchers have proposed that the high correlations are due to a common factor, often called “neuroticism” or “negative affectivity” (Watson & Clark, 1984) The other major factor which has made it difficult to understand and treat anxiety and depressive disorders is their heterogeneity. There appear to be distinct subtypes within each general category which have different implications for etiology and treatment (van Praag et al., 1988; 1990b). The heterogeneity of depressive and anxiety disorders suggest that it may be important to discriminate between different subtypes in order to make sense of paradoxical and inconsistent empirical findings. Because of this heterogeneity, it may be crucial to focus more specifically at the symptom level in order to understand important distinctions and commonalities between the anxiety and depressive disorders. The current study examines whether there is a unique cluster of symptoms describing depression that can be distinguished from a unique cluster of symptoms depicting anxiety. One possibility is that there are in fact two distinct clusters. In that case, one can then evaluate whether widely used self-report instruments represent a good sampling of those symptom clusters uniquely associated with the relevant disorder. A second possibility, suggested by many authors, is that there are more than one cluster defined by symptoms of anxiety or depression, or by symptoms of both of these disorders. In that event, one can evaluate whether those patterns correspond to predictions associated with a number of theoretical views to be discussed regarding the heterogeneity within, and overlap between, anxiety and depression. CHAPTER II THEORETICAL BACKGROUND Many reasons have been suggested to account for the strong relationship between anxiety and depressive disorders. First, a subset of the same symptoms are often used to diagnose both of these disorders. AS a result, many of the instruments used to assess these disorders lack discriminant validity (Gotlib & Cane, 1989). Second, there appear to be common etiological factors, as suggested by family, genetic, and attachment studies. Third, there is evidence of common underlying physiological substrates. Fourth, research on “learned helplessness” has pointed the way to a theory of anxiety driven depression, in which anxiety causes depression (van Praag, 1994). Fifth, assessment issues have complicated attempts to understand these disorders. Finally, there are claims that both anxiety and depression involve maladaptive cognitive processes (Clark & Beck, 1989; Kendall & Ingram, 1989), or alternatively, a general distress factor, which is postulated as the source of the high correlations (Watson et al, 1995a; 1995b) Smptoms Two unipolar depressive disorders are described in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-III-R; American Psychiatric Association, 1987): major depression and dysthymia. The essential features of major depression are depressed mood and/or loss of interest or pleasure. Other symptoms include change in weight or appetite, sleep disturbance, psychomotor retardation or agitation, fatigue, feelings of worthlessness, inappropriate guilt, cognitive deficits such as diminished concentration or indecisiveness, and suicidal ideation. Additional symptoms associated with dysthymia include irritability, low self-esteem, and feelings of hopelessness (American Psychiatric Association, 1987). One essential feature which distinguishes between major depression and dysthymia is duration: in the DSM-III-R, dysthymia is defined as a chronic condition with persistent or intermittent symptoms, of at least two years duration. However, in some ways this last distinction may be somewhat artificial, in light of studies which show high rates of dysthymic-major depressive comorbidity Akiskal & Weise, 1992). Reviews of the relevant literature suggest that dysthymia is both a predisposing factor toward (i.e., a prodromal stage of) and a consequence of major depressive disorder (Akiskal, 1994; Akiskal & Weise, 1992). Several anxiety disorders are described in DSM-III-K including panic disorder, phobias, obsessive compulsive disorder, and generalized anxiety disorder. The symptoms associated with generalized anxiety disorder include unrealistic or excessive anxiety or apprehension, such as fear of dying, going crazy, or of doing something uncontrolled. In DSM-III-R, the remaining symptoms are grouped under three categories: motor tension, autonomic hyperactivity, and vigilance and scanning. Motor tension includes such symptoms as trembling, muscle tension, aches, restlessness, and fatigue. Symptoms of autonomic arousal include shortness of breath, dizziness, palpitations, sweating, trouble swallowing, abdominal distress, flushes or chills, and frequent urination. Symptoms associated with vigilance and scanning include feeling keyed up or on edge, exaggerated startle response, concentration difficulties, difiiculty falling or staying asleep, and irritability. The individual may also experience feelings of depersonalization or derealization. Panic disorder adds unpredictable attacks of panic to the preceding, whereas phobic disorders are characterized by persistent and irrational fear associated with avoidance of the dreaded object or situation. As is evident from the previous descriptions, there is a great deal of overlap among symptoms of depression and generalized anxiety disorder. Both anxiety and depression may be accompanied by irritability, dysphoric mood, feelings of tension, apprehension, worry, concentration difficulties, fatigue, and self-preoccupation (Gotlib & Crane, 1989; Sarason, 1985; Spielberger, 1972). Torgerson (1985) found that irritability and anger are more common in individuals with symptoms of both anxiety and depression than in either group alone. Unfortunately, he did not distinguish between irritability and anger in his study, and so these results may be confounded. Mook, van der Ploog, and Kleijn (1990) found indications that the high correlations between anxiety, anger and depression may be due largely to the high correlations between anxiety and anger, and anxiety and depression, respectively. There are also important differences between anxiety and depressive disorders. Whereas sadness is most often reported by depressed individuals, fear appears to be the predominant affective experience associated with anxiety (Bartlett & Izard, 1972; Izard, Blumber, & Oyster, 1985; Ollendick & Yule, 1990). In a review of descriptive studies, Breier, Chamey and Heninger (1985) found that, across studies, the symptoms which best discriminated depression from anxiety were depressed mood, early morning awakening, suicidal ideation, and psychomotor retardation. The symptoms which best discriminated anxiety fiom depression were panic attacks, agoraphobia, and compulsive features (Breier et al., 1985). Loss of interest or pleasure, and pessimism have also been noted to discriminate between these disorders (Clark, 1989). These latter findings are consistent with a recent review of studies with children and adolescents, in which Brady and Kendall (1992) found that symptoms of depression, such as anhedonia and low self-esteem, discriminated depression from anxiety. In contrast, symptoms of anxiety were less usefirl in discriminating between groups (Brady & Kendall, 1992). This may be due, at least in part, to overlap of items between scales in frequently used inventories for children. DiaLesis of Anxietyand Depressive Disorders Comorbidity Clinical studies afiirm that the high correlations between anxiety and depression are not merely due to problems with self -report. High comorbidity rates using diagnostic criteria have been interpreted by some to imply a common diathesis for these disorders. The diagnosis of depression is often a secondary diagnosis among patients with anxiety disorders (Barlow, 1985, Kashani et al., 1987). In a cross-sectional study, researchers found strong associations between anxiety and depression (Angst, Vollrath, Merikangas, & Ernst, 1990). They found the strongest association between panic disorder and 1 depression: comorbidity rates were five times higher than would be expected by chance. Longitudinal findings from the Angst and colleagues (1990) study are consistent with other studies which have suggested that symptoms of anxiety are more likely to precede manifestations of depression in individuals who develop symptoms of both disorders (Schatzberg et al., 1990). Angst and his colleagues (1990) found that 62% of those individuals who were eventually diagnosed with both disorders first manifested symptoms of anxiety, compared to 18% who initially manifested symptoms of depression. Pure depression tended to remain stable across the 7 year follow-up period, whereas individuals initially diagnosed with only anxiety tended to manifest symptoms of depression by follow-up. Although these differences were not Statistically significant, they may still be meaningfirl; the small sample sizes in clinical studies tend to favor statistical Type II errors. In the Angst and colleagues (1990) study, half of the purely anxious subjects developed major depression or recurrent brief depression during the follow-up period. This is consistent with other reports which have shown a greater tendency for individuals with anxiety disorders to develop symptoms of depression than for depressed individuals to develop symptoms of anxiety (I-Iagnell & Grasbeck, 1990). Family Studies Family studies suggest a common diathesis for anxiety and mood disorders. These disorders tend to co-occur in families as well as in individuals. Panic disorder, but not generalized anxiety disorder, appears to have a high specific family prevalence and genetic transmission (Breier, Chamey, & Heninger, 1985; Maier, Buller, & Hallmayer, 1988). However, there have been inconsistent reports in the literature as to the heritability and etiology of mood and anxiety disorders. This may be due, in part, to the practice in some family studies of employing diagnostic exclusion criteria. Studies which have employed exclusion criteria have not supported a relationship between anxiety disorders and depression (Crowe, Noyes, Pauls, & Slymen, 1983), whereas studies which have ignored diagnostic exclusion criteria have supported a strong relationship between panic disorder and major depression, in particular (Leckman, Weissman, Merikangas, Pauls, & Prusoff, 1983). When rates from previous studies which had employed exclusion criteria were recalculated to include secondary depressive disorders, the relationship between anxiety and depression was again supported (Clark, 1989). In a review of family and genetic studies of parental depression and child psychopathology, Weissman (1990) reported that children of depressed parents are at increased risk for diagnosis of both mood and anxiety disorders. She found no statistically significant differences in frequencies of depression and anxiety in children of parents who had been diagnosed with a mood or anxiety disorder. No significant difi‘erences in transmission of anxiety and depression have been found in children even when diagnosis and comorbidity of parents was taken into account (W eissman, Leckman, Merikangas, Gammon, & Prusoff, 1984). However, the small sample sizes in clinical studies often make it difiicult to detect meaningful differences. Studies which have examined the relatives of children with anxiety and depressive disorders have found high rates of both depression and anxiety disorders. In contrast to the previous studies, these studies do show specificity of transmission. For example, when Puig-Antich and Rabinovich (1986) examined the rates of major depression in relatives of proband children, they found that 39% of anxious children and 55% of depressed children had relatives with major depression (See Table 1). In another study, looking at rates of disorder in mothers of children diagnosed with anxiety disorders, 77.4% of the mothers were diagnosed with anxiety disorders, whereas 42.2 % of the mothers were diagnosed with major depression (Last, Francis, & Hersen, Kazdin, & Strauss, 1987). Table 1 Rates of Anxiety and Depressive Disorders in Children and Relatives CHILD ADULT DIAGNOSIS DIAGNOSIS ANXH’ZTY DEPRESSION ANXIETY 77% 39% DEPRESSION 42% 5 5% In conclusion, the evidence fi'om family studies shows a strong relationship between depression and mixed anxiety and depressive disorders. There are conflicting results regarding the specificity of the transmission of these disorders, which may best be resolved by a thorough meta-analysis of the relevant studies. Generalized anxiety has been linked to particularly high rates (70%) of secondary depression. These findings have been taken to indicate that this disorder may be more highly associated with a “general distress” factor than are more specific anxiety disorders (Dohrenwend, 1990; Noyes, Clarkson, 10 Crowe, Yates, & McChesney, 1987). However, there are also reports of links between panic disorders and depressive symptoms, which would support a “spectrum” model of the mood disorders, in which anxiety is included as one pole of mood dysregulation, with depressed mood describing the opposite pole (Lopez-Ibor, 1990). “Kindling” models suggest that over time the specificity or severity of stressors becomes less important in producing symptoms (Gold, Goodwin, & Chrousos, 1988). Twin studies Twin studies also point to similarities in etiology, and provide a better opportunity to differentiate between genetic and environmental factors implicated in the etiology of anxiety and depressive disorders. Twin studies have suggested that differences between individuals in measures of anxiety and depression can best be explained by differences in genes and individual environmental experiences, rather than shared environmental experiences (Jardine, Martin, & Henderson, 1984). Early studies suggested that there were underlying nonspecific hereditary factors which may lead to a predisposition to both anxiety and depressive disorders (Clifford, Hopper, Fulker, & Murray, 1984; Jardine, Martin, & Henderson, 1984), as well as to neuroticism, more generally (Andrews, Stewart, Allen, & Henderson, 1990). However, recent advances in behavior genetics have provided new methods for testing some of these models. Carey and DiLalla (1994) reanalyzed data fi'om Eaves, Eysenck, and Martin (1989) in an attempt to evaluate causal models regarding neuroticisnr, anxiety, and depression. Their results did not support a causal link from neuroticism to anxiety or depression, nor did it support the existence of a 11 common higher order factor linking these three constructs. In a review of recent twin studies, Torgerson (1990) pointed out that, in addition to the common genetic factors which increase susceptibility to symptoms of both anxiety and depression, there are also genetic factors which are only linked to specific symptoms of anxiety. For example, Martin, Jardine, Andrews, and Heath (1988) found that feelings of panic appear to be shaped by genetic influences which do not affect other symptoms associated with anxiety. Aflchment Perspectives. Research has shown a correlation between the quality of early interactions between parent and child and later development (Murray & Trevarthen, 1985). These correlations have been taken as causal; however, most of the research fails to control for genetics. According to attachment theorists, the caretaker moderates the young child’s experience, keeping the child from becoming overwhelmed by strong affect. Over time, the child takes over more and more of these regulatory functions. Early interpersonal experiences become the framework for understanding both self and other via internal representations or ‘working models’ which guide expectations and actions (Bretherton, 1985). By the end of the first year of life, the child has begun to develop complex working models of human interaction (Kraemer, Ebert, Schmidt, & McKinney, 1991), which facilitate the development of affective self-regulation and the modulation of impulses (Schwalbe, 1991). These models of relationships are believed by many to have longstanding ramifications for the quality of later experiences as well as the ability to moderate affect in later years, with 12 particular implications for anxiety and depression (Kobak & Sceery, 1988; Kobak, Sudler, & Gamble, 1992). Recent advances in neurobiological research suggest that adverse early experiences not only impede the individual’s ability to moderate their experiences, they also have structural implications for the developing organism; interactions which are responsive to the child’s needs appear to facilitate the normal physiological development of the neural structures which underlie affective modulation and well-being (Schore, 1994). Adverse early experiences may impair the individual’s ability to self-regulate affective experiences, leaving the individual more vulnerable to becoming overwhelmed by strong affects such as sadness, shame, or fear. The ability to self-regulate affect efficiently allows the individual to cope with stress with fewer costs (Schmale & Engel, 1975). The underlying structure which appears to be critical in the development, storage, and regulation of internal representations linked to the regulation of affective information appears to be dopaminergic (Joseph, 1988; Schore, 1994). This is consistent with research which links deficits in goal seeking behaviors (Swerdlow & Koob, 1987) and “positive emotionality” (Depue, Luciana, Arbisi, Collins, & Leon, 1994) to the dopaminergic system (Swerdlow & Koob, 1987), suggesting that this monoamine may be particularly important in depressions in which anhedonia or psychomotor retardation is a major symptom (van Praag, 1980b). Chrousos and Gold (1992) have delineated two distinct forms of stress system dysregulation: one associated with hyperarousal, and the other taking the form of hypoarousal. These two responses to acute stress are consistent with the vigilance and 13 arousal activation associated with anxiety, on the one hand, and the psychomotor retardation, fatigue, and anhedonia linked to depression, on the other. Physiological Studies Evidence from epidemiological and animal studies biochemically links anxiety and depression. Findings suggest that indices of noradrenergic and neuroendocrine function may be disturbed in both disorders, albeit somewhat differently (Leckman, Weissman, Merikangas, Pauls, & Prusoff, 1983; van Praag, 1994). Some variants of each disorder respond to monoamine oxidase (MAO) inhibitors and tricyclic antidepressants (Uhde, Roy-Byme, Vittone, Boulenger, & Post, 1985; Weissman, Leckman, Merikangas, Gammon, & Prusoff, 1984). This is not true, however, of generalized anxiety disorder, which, unlike obsessive compulsive disorder or panic disorder, does not respond to MAO inhibitors or tricyclic antidepressants. There is also evidence which suggests that it may be possible to distinguish between subtypes of depression on the basis of differential neurophysiological underpinnings. For example, Weiss and Sirnson (1985) suggest that anxiety which occurs in the face of uncontrollable stressors produces an accompanying depressive state. This type of depression has been conceptualized as “anxious depression” (Weiss & Simson, 1985) or “S-HT (serotonin)-related, anxiety-driven depression” (van Praag, 1994). It is more likely that specific symptoms or clusters of symptoms, rather than complex syndromes such as anxiety or depression, will be linked to specific transmitter systems. For example, van Praag and his colleagues (1990a; 1990b) have connected the l4 initiation and maintenance of goal directed behaviors to the dopaminergic system, hedonic firnctions associated with reward coupling to the norepinephrine system, and the affective regulation of aggression and anxiety to the serotonergic system. Viewing symptoms in this way helps to clarify why drugs which affect the serotonergic system have been found to be useful in the treatment of both anxiety and depressive disorders, in both of which the modulation of arousal may be an important component, and may be experienced either as hostility, or anxiety. Van Praag (1994) later noted that serotonin related drugs work poorly with some patients; Katz and his colleagues (1994) noted that about one third of depressed patients do not respond to these drugs. Clinical studies help point to ways in which symptoms of anxiety and depression are tied together at the neurotransmitter level, but can be more clearly delineated when the components are broken down into discrete symptom clusters. For example, Katz and his colleagues (1994) found that biochemical changes in the serotonergic system were more strongly linked to mood aspects of depression, such as hostility and anxiety, whereas changes in the noradrenergic system were more strongly linked to behavioral aspects of depression associated with psychomotor retardation and arousal, such as anxiety, agitation, and somatic symptoms. Katz and his colleagues (1994) found very different results when considering unipolar versus bipolar responders to the intervention under study. In unipolar responders lower levels of norepinephrine were associated with lower levels of hostility and smaller decreases of norepinephrine were associated with reductions in psychomotor retardation. 15 Smaller decreases of a serotonin (S-hydroxyindoleacetic acid (5-HIAA); the major metabolite of serotonin) and dopamine metabolite were associated with decreased anxiety. Interestingly, smaller decreases in the norepinephrine metabolite were associated with more positive outcomes, suggesting that it may be crucial to look at actual neurotransmitter levels rather than increases or decreases per se. Bipolar responders showed a somewhat different pattern: smaller decreases in norepinephrine were also associated with decreased motor retardation. However, lower levels of 5-HIAA were associated with decreased depressed mood, and greater levels of the dopamine metabolite were associated with decreased hostility (Katz et. al., 1994). These results support other studies which have affirmed the importance of considering subtype when looking at mood disorders. These findings also support earlier suggestions (Katz et. al., 1987) of a stronger link between the serotonergic system and anxiety than with dysphoric mood. There is still controversy whether decreases in 5-HIAA in the cerebrospinal fluid is a reflection of enhanced serotonergic transmission (Ericksson & Humble, 1990; Meltzer, 1990). If this link does exist, it would make sense of differences between unipolars and bipolars in the therapeutic action of serotonergic drugs which have been reported (Katz et al., 1994). For bipolars, greater reductions in a S-HIAA were associated with reductions in anxiety and depressed mood, whereas for unipolars, smaller reductions in S-HIAA were associated with reductions in anxiety (Katz et al., 1994). This suggests that the role of the serotonergic system is different in unipolar depression, or else that it is not relevant to the 16 therapeutic action of the drug. In this context, it is notable that anxiety appears to play a larger role in unipolar than in bipolar depression (Katz, Robins, Croughan, Secunda, & Swann, 1982). Serotonergic drugs tend to be more effective in the treatment of disorders in which anxiety plays a major role, than in those in which psychomotor disturbance is prevalent (Deakin, Guimaraes, Wang, & Hensman, 1991;1nsel, 1991). These findings affirm the importance of focusing on symptom clusters when trying to understand important differences between anxiety and depression. Drug trials support the importance of looking at specific subtypes or symptom clusters of anxiety. Consistent with results from family and genetic studies, drug treatment studies support panic disorder, obsessive compulsive disorder, and generalized anxiety disorder as separate nosological entities (Heninger & Chamey, 1988). The literature exploring the concomitants of learned helplessness extends our understanding of some of the physiological underpinnings of anxiety and depressive symptomatology by looking at the effects of chronic or uncontrollable stress. The behavioral deficits associated with learned helplessness include the anhedonia, helplessness, and despair often associated with depressive disorders (Maier & Seligrnan, 1976; Schutz, Schutz, Orsingher, & Izquierdo, 1979). Severe stress leads to monoamine dysregulation which includes the dopaminergic and noradrenergic systems. In an attempt to better delineate the functions of these two monoamines, Dubovsky (1993) linked underlying neurological substrates to specific psychobiological firnctions, rather than to discrete diagnostic syndromes, and found that the noradrenergic 17 system is most often associated with arousal, orientation to danger, alerting, learning, memory, and sympathetic nervous system functioning. Symptoms associated with noradrenergic dysregulation include agitation, arousal, fearfirlness, vigilance, insomnia, and withdrawal (Dubovsky, 1993). Dubovsky links the dopaminergic system to movement, reward, and motivation. Symptoms associated with dopanrinergic dysregulation include the psychomotor retardation, anhedonia, helplessness, and despair often associated with depression (Swerdlow & Koob, 1987). Serotonergic dysfirnction has been linked to anxiety, aggression, motivation, memory, skeletal muscle function, as well as to regulatory firnctions such as mood, sleep, appetite, body temperature, and sexual behaviors (Dubovsky, 1993; Cloninger, 1986; van Praag et al., 1990a) . Associated symptomatology includes impulsivity, aggression, suicidality, sadness, anxiety, as well as sleep and appetite disturbances (Apter et al., 1990; Dubovsky, 1993; Soubrie, 1986; van Praag, 1990b). “Learned Helplessness” and “Serotonin Driven Degemn” The best defined distinct subtype of unipolar depression is a type called “serotonin driven depression” by van Praag (1994). This form of depression was originally hypothesized from results on experiments called “learned helplessness” experiments. This section reviews that research, which strongly links anxiety and depression. The reader is warned that the label “learned helplessness” is now known to be a very misleading label for “learned helplessness” experiments. 18 The Label “Learned Helplessness” The original “learned helplessness” experiments took on the following form. Animals were exposed to uncontrolled and inescapable stress. Following this stress, the animals showed performance deficits in escape learning. For example, consider a situation in which untreated animals easily learn a response to escape shock. Following stress, many of these animals were unable to learn the escape response, or they were very slow in learning it. That is, they acted “helpless” in the face of shock. Researchers early on noted that this is similar to the pattern shown by severely depressed patients (Maier & Seligman, 1976; Schutz, Schutz, Orsingher, & Izquierdo, 1979). The “learned helplessness” experiments have subsequently been used as animal models for depression, although there have also been human studies done within the “learned helplessness” paradigm. The label “learned helplessness” was coined by certain theorists who had formed a similar theory for depression disorders. This theory claims that depression is caused by feelings of helplessness. These feelings are claimed to be produced by a belief in the lack of contingency between one’s actions and potential outcomes (Gerber, Miller, & Seaman, 1979). Others (Abramson, Metalsky, & Alloy, 1989) have further claimed that feelings of helplessness are caused by self-blame or “internal” attributions for negative events. These theorists interpret the findings of the “learned helplessness” experiments as showing that people learn helplessness from self-blame attributions for stress. The problem with the label “learned helplessness” is that it is got learned (c.f. Paul, 1988, p. 15 ). There are several forms of evidence which show this, including findings on A l9 duration and findings on the induction of learned helplessness by drugs rather than by stress (Weiss & Simson, 1985). Consider duration: Learning without counterlearning lasts for periods of days, months, and years. The behavior deficits for “learned helplessness” experiments wear off completely in 72 hours and are largely gone in 24 hours. Furthermore, Weiss and his colleagues (1981) have noted that effects past 4 hours are probably due to conditioned anxiety which reproduces the anxiety state, which then reproduces the depressed state. Consider newer studies on the induction of “learned helplessness” behavior deficits using other drugs: Petty, Kramer, and Moeller (1994) reviewed studies showing that “learned helplessness” can be induced by injections of anxiogenic drugs, such as haloperidol. It can be induced by other anxiety producing drugs, as well, such as benzodiazapine receptor ligands (Drugan, Maier, Skolnick, Paul, & Crawley, 1985). That is, there need be no stress manipulation to get the effects of “learned helplessness.” “Learned Helpless_ness” finding The key finding of the “learned helplessness” studies can be restated as this: A state of high anxiety produces a delayed state of high depression, in which the subject feels dysphoria, irritability, and anhedonia. The anhedonia eliminates the emotional effects of reinforcement (Ettenberg, 1989) and thus makes it hard to learn from successfirl experiences, such as the relief from fear produced by a successfirl escape experience. Anhedonia also makes it hard to elicit previously learned responses that were rewarded by positive reinforcement. 20 There have also been many studies done on the brain chemistry of the depression produced in “learned helplessness” experiments. These Studies suggest that the serotonin effects produced by anxiety result in depleted norepinephrine in the locus ceruleus area of the brain (Paul, 1988), and that it is this depletion which produces the anhedonia of the depressed state. Anxiety driven depression The data in “learned helplessness” studies show that a state of high anxiety will produce a state of high depression (Barlow, 1991). That in itself says nothing about trait anxiety and trait depression. But consider the implications of this finding for people with high state anxiety. A person with high state anxiety often experiences states of intense anxiety. Each such experience produces a state of depression which lasts about 4 hours. Thus, a person who experiences a high rate of intense state anxiety will automatically experience a high rate of associated depression. Van Praag (19803) made this inference at an early date in this line of research and concluded fiom this that there should be a subtype of depression caused by serotonin dysregulation. In later reports, he thought that data had disconfirmed this hypothesis (van Praag et al., 1990a). However, in van Praag (1994), he pulled together all the data and showed strong support for this hypothesis. Depressed patients who show low levels of serotonin metabolites in their cerebral spinal fluid are much more likely to respond positively to antidepressants than patients whose first presenting symptoms are of anxiety. Those whose first and only symptoms are those of depression usually do not respond to 811‘ 581'! asp WOU 21 tricyclic antidepressants. Furthermore, there is a temporal progression to the recovery. They first show a reduction in anxiety and then later Show a reduction in depression. The one key difference between the van Praag (1994) theory and the simple anxiety-depression hypothesis is that van Praag noted the importance of hostility and aggression to this theory. Serotonin has an even stronger influence on hostility and aggression than on anxiety. Van Praag noted that in the studies done, hostility was equally a part of the pattern. That is, in the patients who appear to be serotonin driven depressives, hostility was an early presenting factor just as often as anxiety. In addition, hostility - like anxiety - showed early reduction in the patients who responded positively to tricyclics. The van Praag theory is closely related to the epidemiological findings of an asymmetry in the development of anxiety and depressive disorders. That is, those who first develop anxiety fi'equently go on to develop either firll or partial depressive disorders. Those who first develop depression usually do ao_t go on to develop problems with anxiety. Those who develop depression following anxiety disorders would be the serotonin driven depressives. Those who develop depression first and who do not develop anxiety problems are a second type of depressive: those not likely to respond to serotonin-relatedantidepressant drugs. It would be interesting to know if hostility/aggression problems show the same asymmetry in time for aggression as that found for anxiety. The van Praag (1994) theory would predict this. However, the epidemiological studies rarely report on hostility and 22 aggression problems. Quantitative Theories of Anxietyaand Depression Quantitative theories of state and trait depression consider comparisons rather than extreme cases. Consider then two people, one of whom is higher than the other in trait anxiety. The person who is higher in trait anxiety will be more likely to experience states of intense anxiety which will produce the accompanying state of depression. Thus, the person who is higher in trait anxiety will also be higher in trait depression. If all depression were caused by serotonin dysregulation, then there would be a near perfect correlation between trait anxiety and trait depression, even though the two are conceptually distinct entities. However, the studies reviewed by van Praag (1994) also show that there are depressed patients who do not respond well to tricyclics and who show a very different temporal pattern in the development of depression. The implication for the joint relationship of anxiety and depression is this: The data should Show that as anxiety goes up, depression goes up. However, as anxiety goes down, depression need not disappear. Rather, those whose depression is not caused by serotonin dysregulation could Show up as people with low anxiety, but high depression. Thus, the contingency table for anxiety and depression would be predicted to show an asymmetry: (a) no cases of high anxiety without high depression, but (b) many cases of high depression without high anxiety. 23 Assessment An underlying dilemma in defining depression and anxiety has been the multiple meanings of these terms, ranging from affective experiences, to syndromes, to disorders. Many theorists now view each of these disorders along a continuum extending from common affective experiences to diagnosable disorders (Beck & Clark, 1988). Much of the clinical, family, and psychophysiological literature supports this view (Paul, 1988; van Praag et al., 1988). Quantitative studies find no break in the distribution of anxiety and no break in the distribution of depression. Self-report instruments. Most anxiety self-report instruments describe features of generalized anxiety disorder (Gotlib & Cane, 1989). Most depression instruments describe symptoms associated with unipolar depression (Gotlib & Cane, 1989). Many anxiety and depression scales correlate between .70 and .90 with other scales measuring the same construct, which would provide good evidence for convergent validity. Correlations between constructs of between .40 and .60 (with a range between .27 and .94; Clark, Beck, & Stewart, 1990) have been reported. Gotlib and Cane (1989) have interpreted the high correlation between anxiety and depression in self-report instruments as a lack of validity in self-report. As mentioned previously, there is considerable overlap between symptoms. If the high correlation were due to overlapping symptoms in the scales, that high correlation might be interpreted as poor discriminative validity. However, there is also high comorbidity between these two disorders. Thus, it is not clear whether the high 24 correlation between anxiety and depressive instruments is due to poor discriminant validity or to the high comorbidity rates for these disorders (Regier, Burke, & Burke, 1990). Mountjoy and Roth (1982) found that depression was more often described as persistent in depressive patients, whereas it was more often described as mild or episodic in patients with a primary diagnosis of anxiety. There were no significant differences between anxious and depressed patients in their self-reports of tension. Some people have accused depressives of exaggerating reports of level of symptomatology and general distress (Prusoff & Klerrnan, 1974). Kelly and Walter (1969) found that agitated depressives rated their own anxiety as more severe than did anxious patients whose autonomic arousal levels were higher. Even non-agitated depressed patients rated their anxiety as high as did anxious patients, in spite of physiological evidence to the contrary. However, it should be noted that autonomic arousal is not the same as fear and vigilance; a depressed individual’s experience of ruminative anxiety may indeed be more severe to that individual even though there may be a lower level of autonomic arousal. Common Concomitants Personality Factors Another method for understanding the comorbidity of anxiety and depressive disorders is by looking at commonalities and differences in personality characteristics. Cloninger (1987; Cloninger, Martin, Guze, & Clayton, 1990) has delineated three dimensions of personality: novelty seeking, described as impulsive versus constrained; ha rer ass ho: 19? de; per due rnor fion diso Cleve Ofier <fifltt beha aSan dePre to int: 25 harm avoidance, described as apprehensive or cautious versus fearless or uninhibited; and reward dependence, described as sensitive to social cues versus detached. Strong associations have been found between harm avoidance and negative mood states, such as hostility, anxiety, depression, fatigue, and confirsion (Svrakic, Przybeck, & Cloninger, 1992). Cloninger (1986) contends that individuals high in harm avoidance and reward dependence, and also low on novelty seeking, are more likely to develop either anxiety or depressive disorders (1986; 1988a). Cloninger (1986) suggests that similarities in personality types account for the high comorbidity of these two disorders (1986; 1988a). There is evidence that the overlap between these two disorders may be primarily due to secondary features; depression is often a sequelae of chronic psychopathology more generally (Cloninger, Martin, Guze, & Clayton, 1990). There are some indications from family studies that there may be little overlap between primary anxiety and depressive disorders, but rather, individuals with anxiety disorders may be at higher risk for developing secondary depression (Cloninger, Martin, Guze, & Clayton, 1981). Some theorists have suggested that there is a general subjective distress factor, often termed "neuroticism," which has been defined as ”a broad dimension of individual differences in the tendency to experience negative, distressing emotions and to possess behavioral and cognitive traits" (Costa & McCrae, 1987, p. 301). Neuroticism is viewed as an enduring emotional instability, and correlates .59 with anxiety and .51 with depression in trait measures (Eysenck & Eysenck, 1968). These correlations are difiicult to interpret. There is no independent way to measure neuroticism; the scales which 26 purport to do so are largely saturated with anxiety and depression items, along with other associated symptoms, such as hostility. Recovered anxious and depressed patients have been found to be more neurotic than control subjects (Reich, Noyes, Hirschfield, Coryell, & O'Gorman, 1987). “Neuroticism” is also a strong predictor of chronicity (Hirschfield, Klerman, Andreasen, Clayton, & Keller, 1986). In a high-risk study, Hirschfield and his colleagues (1989) found that factors associated with neuroticism, such as decreased emotional stability and poorer accommodation to stressfirl situations predicted later depressive episodes. However, the high recidivism rates for depression suggest that these individuals may be moving from major depressive episodes to dysthymia; “recovery” may mean alleviation of acute symptoms (Akiskal & Weise, 1992). Not surprisingly, neuroticism, much like anxiety and depression, appears to be largely genetically determined (Henderson, 1982), and genetic factors also appear to be important in predicting the covariation of neuroticism, depression, and anxiety (Jardine, Martin, & Henderson, 1984; Martin, Jardine, Andrews, & Heath, 1988). More recently, this purported tendency to experience negative emotional states has been conceptualized as a pervasive mood disposition, and has been termed "negative affectivity" (Watson & Clark, 1984). Negative affectivity is measured by symptoms of negative affective states including nervousness, tension, anger, guilt, and sadness. Individuals high in negative affectivity are those who have a heightened sensitivity to life stressors and a negative self-image (Watson & Clark, 1984). Watson and Clark (1984) h. 27 claim that it is positive affectivity, or the disposition to experience positive affective states, which may best discriminate between anxiety and depression. In contrast to Watson and Clark, whose conceptualization of both depression and anxiety includes high negative affectivity, Tellegen (1985) has conceptualized only anxiety as high negativity, positing depression as a deficit in positive affectivity. In this way, Tellegen is viewing depression as anhedonia, one predominant symptom of the disorder. Tellegen's view is consistent with indications from the literature that mild depression is more strongly defined by a lack of positive self-evaluations than with a predominance of negative self-evaluations (Beck & Clark, 1991). Maladaptive Cognitive Processes Empirical findings link personality characteristics associated with negative temperament to poor outcomes, such as lower self-esteem (Block & Robins, 1993 ), anxiety, and depression (Watson & Clark, 1984). This may be due, at least in part, to overlap between items in these scales. There are indications that cognitive states can have important implications for affective experiences. For example, self-focus can intensify affective states (Gibbons, 1991). Depression includes a tendency to perseverate on negative events, increasing the experience of negative, but not positive, affect. Alternatively, there are also indications that the absence of positive focus may be more focal than the presence of a negative focus (Beck & Clark, 1991). The rumination and negative expectations often associated with depression may replace active engagement and constructive problem solving. Cognitive models also provide a means for understanding 28 how anxiety may lead to depression; Carver and Scheier (1991) suggest that when feelings of anxiety disrupt attention, positive versus negative expectations determine whether or not the activity is resumed. Within this framework, a paucity of favorable expectancies leads to disengagement. Both anxiety and depression are characterized by maladaptive cognitive processes and irrational beliefs. However, there is evidence that the cognitive content of maladaptive cognitions may differ between depressed and anxious individuals (Beck, Brown, Eidelson, Steer, & Riskind, 1987). In anxiety, maladaptive cognitions tend to have an orientation toward a firture in which threat or harm is anticipated, whereas in depression the focus tends to be toward past perceived losses, failures, or degradation. These differences in orientation also appear to have implications for how individuals process information. The anxious individual may be hypervigilant for signs of threat or harm, overestimate the possibility of danger, and tend to recall information which is associated with threat or anxiety. In contrast, the depressive may tend to differentially process and recall information associated with loss and failure. Depressed individuals also tend to discount positive self-referential information (Kendall & Ingram, 1989). These types of irrational beliefs and maladaptive cognitions may also be important in helping us to understand and distinguish between distinct subtypes of depression. Anaclitic and Introjective Depression In his original and theoretical work, Blatt (1974) claimed that depressed people can be split into two types: anaclitic and introjective depressives. However, when he 29 moved into research, there was a subtle shift in theories: a shift from types to “styles.” This shift obscured the problems for the original theory. The data seem to contradict the original theory. This section will present the original theory. The empirical work will be briefly reviewed with an emphasis on the two scales for anaclitic and introjective depression. Selected findings for the scales will be reviewed. This evidence seems to disconfirrn the original type theory. The evidence may disconfirrn the use of the term “depression” in naming the scales. Finally, some hypotheses for this research will be framed. B_latt’s Theory: Subtypes of Depresm Blatt (1974) hypothesized that there are two types of depressives: anaclitic and introjective depressives. His theory is developmental. Depression can be produced fiom either of two kinds of experience: (a) helplessness and dependency or (b) feelings of inferiority and resulting self-criticism. He posited two types of depressive. First, the anaclitic depressive is characterized by feelings of helplessness, weakness, and dependency. Second, the introjective depressive, presumed to be developmentally more advanced, is characterized by feelings of inferiority, guilt, and fears relating to perceived failure to live up to standards or expectations. Many authors have focused on this dichotomy in their attempts to understand depression (Arieti & Bemporad, 1980; Blatt & Zuroff, 1992; Grinker, Miller, Sabshin, Nunn, & Nunnally, 1961; Pilkonis, 1988). 3. ar 111 de all 3O BlatL’a Reaearch: Styles Blatt devised an instrument using items which were constructed to reflect experiences often reported by depressed individuals, and were not specifically associated with any particular theoretical orientation (Blatt, D’Afilitti, & Quinlan, 1976a). A factor analysis of these items results in three factors. The anaclitic, or dependent, style was described as externally directed with regard to interpersonal relations. The themes included abandonment, loneliness, helplessness, rejection, dependency, and managing negative affect to avoid the loss of the other. The introjective, or self-critical style, was described as more internally directed. The themes included guilt, emptiness, hopelessness, insecurity, dissatisfaction with self, self-blame, failure to meet standards or expectations, and being threatened by change or responsibility. The third factor was identified as “efficacy,” with no recognition of the fact that self-efficacy and self-criticism should be negatively correlated with each other. This third factor consisted of items which indicated a sense of confidence regarding one’s inner resources and capacities. The themes included a focus on high standards, personal responsibility, a sense of autonomy, and pride in one’s accomplishments. The key output fi'om this research effort is two scales: one scale to measure anaclitic depression and the second scale to measure introjective depression. However, there is a key element missing from that research: no cutoff scores to identify the two depressive types. It may be that the researchers looked at the distributions of scale scores and saw no natural break in that distribution. Thus, they may have been hesitant to choose 31 such a point. However, there is also no natural break in the distribution of scores on depression scales either: no break in scores for self-report scales such as the Beck Depression Inventory and no break in scores for psychiatric interview scales such as the Hamilton Depression Scale. Nonetheless, usefirl cutoff scores have been developed for research and practice in regard to depression. In discussing research, Blatt and his colleagues frequently make a subtle, but important, shift in language: a shift from the language of types to the heavy use of the word “styles.” This seems to be an indirect recognition of a shift from considering types to the consideration of quantitative dimensions. However, when this shift is done implicitly, there can be many errors in logic. Type theories are very different in content from dimension theories. The Correlation Between Anaclitigarnd Introjective Depressioa Perhaps the most important finding for the Anaclitic and Introjective depression scales is the fact that the two scales are positively correlated. This would seem to contradict the original theory of two types of depression. In the original theory, there are three types of people: AD= anaclitic depressives ID = introjective depressives ND = non-depressives. 32 Consider the measurement theory behind the development of the Anaclitic depression scale: Anaclitic depression scale ranges: High: AD persons Low: ID and ND persons Consider the measurement theory behind the development of the Introjective depression scale: Introjective depression scale ranges: High: ID persons Low: AD and ND persons In a contingency table for the two scales considered together, we should find the entries depicted in Table 2. Table 2 Contingency Table for the Anaclitic and Introjective Scales Introjective Depression i Anaclitic Low High D es '0 epr sr n High AD Low ND ID The number of people in each cell would depend on the severity of depression used to define the depressed type and it would depend on the population studied. In clinical 33 terms, the level of severity may be defined by whether “minor” depression should be considered or only major depression (“minor” depression is n_ot minor in terms of either impairment or prognosis as noted by Akiskal and Weise, 1992). Consider the general population. If 10% of the population were considered as suffering from serious depression, and if the two types of depression were equally likely, then the table for 100 people would look like the contingency table presented in Table 3. Table 3 Proposed Proportions of Anaclitic and Introjective Depression in the General Population per 100 People Introjective Depression Low High Anaclitic , Depression H1311 5 Low 90 5 Correlation: r = -.05. That is, in a general population, we would expect a low negative correlation between the scales of about 1; = -.05. On the other hand, consider a patient population. If we considered only depressed people to begin with, the contingency table for 100 people would be as shown in Table 4. be qu 5U; 5111 Th 81( 34 Table 4 Proposed Proportions of Anaclitic and Introjective Depression in a Patient Population per 100 Depressed People Introjective Depression Low High Anaclitic , Depression H‘gh 50 Low .. 50 Correlation: r= -1.00 In a population of depressed patients, we would expect a perfect negative correlation between the scales of r = -l .00. Many studies are done with less well defined “outpatient” samples. The key question is: how many depressives are in the particular outpatient sample? For example, suppose that in the outpatient population, 40% of the patients are depressives. That is, suppose that in the outpatient population, 83% of the non-depressives are screened out. The contingency table would look like Table 5. That is, in an outpatient population with 40% depressed patients, we would expect a low negative correlation between the scales of r = -.25. 35 Table 5 Proposed Proportions of Anaclitic and Introjective Depression in an Outpatient Population in Which There are 40% Depressives Introjective Depression Low High Anaclitic . Depression H‘gh 20 Low 60 20 Correlation: r= -.25 To summarize; in the general population, anaclitic and introjective depression should have a low negative correlation of about r = -.05. In a population with only depressed patients, that negative correlation would be a perfect .12 = -1.00. In an outpatient population, the correlation would be somewhere between -.05 and -1.00 depending on the percentage of outpatients that are depressed. For an outpatient population with 40% depressed patients, the correlation should be about [ = -.25. Eight studies were located with data on the correlation between the Anaclitic and Introjective style scales. In some studies, the sample was an outpatient sample. In some studies, the sample was a student sample that would be similar to the general population. The Blatt, Quinlan, Chevron, McDonald, & Zuroff (1982) study had both outpatients and students. Those two values are listed separately and will be analyzed as if they were from separate studies. Unfortunately, one of the studies did not fiilly report the correlations. Zuroff, Moskowitz, Wielgus, Powers, and Franko (1983) computed the correlation for 36 three samples, but report the value for only one sample (N = 39; r = .30). The other two correlations were merely described as “less than .15,” so that study could not be used. The studies and the results that could be used are listed in Table 6. The values in this table vary considerably from one study to another. This is expected in small sample studies because of the large sampling error in such studies. Meta-analysis was used to obtain estimates of the distribution of population values. Several analyses were done and the results are presented in Table 7. Table 6 The Correlation Between Anaclitic and Introjective Style Scales as Reported in Various Studies Authors Sample Size Correlation Subjects Brown & Silberschatz, 1989 60 .51 outpatients Klein, Harding, Taylor, & Dickstcin, 1988 132 .27 outpatients Blatt, Quinlan, Chevron, McDonald, & Zuroff, 1982 I97 .30 outpatients 262 .10 students Smith, O’Keeffe, & Jenkins, 1988 188 .04 students Fuhr & Shean, 1992 150 .47 students Shapiro, 1988 l l 1 .26 students McCranie & Bass, 1984 86 -. l 3 students 37 Table 7 Meta-Analysis of Correlations Between Anaclitic and Introjective Style Scales Number of Number of Studies Subjects Mean rho SD rho ALL STUDIES 8 1186 .21 .16 OUTPATIENTS 3 3 89 .32 .02 STUDENTS 5 797 . 15 . 16 In contrast to the predicted negative correlation between scales, the empirical studies have found a positive correlation between the two style scales. The meta-analysis across all 8 studies found an average correlation of r = .21. This finding simply disconfirrns the type theory. There is more variation in the correlations than is explained by sampling error (the standard deviation of population correlations is estimated to be SD = .16). Variation would be predicted by the type theory since the correlation is predicted to be much more highly negative for an outpatient population than for a student population. Separate analyses were done for the two types of populations: outpatients and students. For the outpatient studies, the average correlation is a positive I = .32 with virtually no variation across studies (estimated SD = .02). For the student samples, the average correlation is a positive _r: = .15. That is, the correlation is positive for both populations. The correlation is a relatively low +. 15 for students and a much higher +.32 for outpatients. These 38 findings are completely inconsistent with the type theory. There was one lone study which did find a negative correlation. McCranie and Bass (1984) report a sample correlation of r = -.13 for a sample of N = 86 students. However, this is a very small sample and the sample correlation could be negative by chance. The 95% confidence interval for this study is -.34 to +08. Since the confidence interval does extend into the positive region, it is possible that the negative value is a fluke. If the negative value for this one study is due to sampling error, then all 8 studies Show positive correlations. To summarize; the type theory of anaclitic and introjective depression predicts a negative correlation between the Anaclitic and Introjective depression scales, a low negative correlation for the general population correlation, and a much larger negative correlation for an outpatient population. The fact is that both correlations are positive. The average correlation for students is a positive +. 15, while the average correlation for outpatients is an even higher positive +.32. Thus, there is a massive departure between the scale findings and the implications of the original type theory. Correlations with Depreasfira There is another potential problem for the type theory. Suppose the two scales are scored 0 and 1 for “low” and “high,” respectively. The two scales considered together should perfectly identify depressives (see Table 8). Table 8 Predicted Sums of Levels for Identification of Anaclitic and Introjective Depressives Person Type Level on Anaclitic Level on Introjective Sum of Levels AD 1 0 l + 0 = 1 ID 0 l O + l = 1 ND 0 O O + O = O This table shows that there should be a perfect multiple correlation for depression as predicted fiom the two scales. While there is evidence of positive correlation between each scale and depression scales, there appears to be no check to see if the correlations are as high as required to have a multiple correlation of 1.00. Research Objectives Research is needed to establish the exact relationship between depression and the Anaclitic and Introjective depression scales. Since the two scales are positively correlated, the original type theory cannot be correct. However, there is an alternative theory. Anaclitic style and introjective style might be symptoms of depression. If this were true, it would be consistent with the original anecdotal observations that generated the type theory. However, it would also be true that as symptoms of depression, the two would be expected to be highly correlated with each other. As symptoms rather than types, perfect prediction of depression would no longer be predicted. Consider anaclitic style and introjective style as symptoms. Existing scales for 40 depression have given little consideration to these traits. This may be an error of omission. On the other hand, the two scales do not appear to be equally linked to depression. The two most frequently used depression questionnaires may be the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI; Beck, Ward, Mendelson, Mock, & Erbaugh, 1961) and the lung Depression Scale (Zing, 1972). Both are more highly correlated with the introjective style than with the anaclitic style (Blatt, Quinlan, Chevron, McDonald, & Zuroff, 1982). This could mean that the anaclitic style is not a symptom of depression or it could mean that dependency is a key symptom which has been omitted from current scales. As it happens, dependency might be a symptom for anxiety rather than depression. The Anaclitic scale is more highly correlated with fearfulness and anxiety than is the Introjective scale (Persons, Burns, Perloff, & Miranda, 1993; Zuroff & Mongrain, 1987). Consistent with this, a dependent, or anaclitic, style has been linked to both depression and anxiety disorders in outpatients, whereas the introjective style was only associated with depression (Bagby et al., 1992). Differences Between Anxiety Symptoms Research also suggests that it may be important to distinguish between different types of anxiety. Panic disorder is defined by DSM-II-R as generalized anxiety disorder plus panic attacks. Panic disorder is more highly associated with depression than are generalized anxiety disorders. There are specific symptoms of panic disorder, such as panic attacks, which may facilitate definition. It may be more difficult to distinguish the 41 psychological symptoms of anxiety disorders versus symptoms of depression. There are indications that it may be usefirl to distinguish between cognitive symptoms of anxiety and somatic symptoms of anxiety. Cognitive anxiety is characterized by apprehension and worry associated with specific cues, muscle tension, Shyness, fatiguability, and behavioral inhibition, whereas somatic anxiety is linked to somatic complaints, high autonomic arousal, and impulsivity (Cloninger, 1988b). Factor analytic studies have shown these to be relatively distinct clusters of symptoms, with some different risk factors (Cloninger, 1988b). For example, Cloninger (1986) found that criminality is associated with increased susceptibility to somatic anxiety and decreased susceptibility to cognitive anxiety. Autonomic and somatic symptoms appear to be also useful in distinguishing between anxiety and depressive disorders (King, Margraf, Ehlers, & Maddock, 1986). Summag One way to understand the conflicting reports regarding consistencies versus inconsistencies between anxiety and depression is to look at the themes and specific symptoms associated with each disorder. Some subtypes of depression may be more highly associated with specific symptoms of anxiety than others. For example, somatic symptoms have been more highly linked to anxiety than to depression (King, Margraf, Ehlers, & Maddock, 1986), and yet they have also been linked to a dependent depressive style (Beck, Epstein, & Harrison, 1983). It is important to remember that the “disorders” under consideration in this study are created, to a large extent, by our attempts to classify, 42 understand, and treat individuals who present with a diverse array of symptoms. These classification structures have evolved along with our understanding, which is always somewhat arbitrary and limited by our preconceptions and limitations. Persons (1986) noted the importance of first delineating the actual psychological phenomena under study in order to be able to build theories which account for those phenomena. In this study, an attempt is being made to delineate the symptoms, or themes, associated with anxiety and depression in order to better understand their interrelationships. CHAPTER III PLAN OF ANALYSIS In this study I will review symptoms of anxiety and depression described in the DSM-III-R, ICD-9, Diagnostic Interview Survey, Research Diagnostic Criteria, and NEO-PI, along with depictions by theorists, such as Beck (1967; Beck, Epstein, Brown, & Steer, 1988), Blatt (1974), Nurcombe and his colleague (1988), Gotlib & Cane (1989), Spielberger (1972), Izard (Bartlett & Izard, 1972; Izard, Blumberg, & Oyster, 1985), Watson and Clark (1984; Clark, 1989), and Tellegen (1985). I will also compile a list of symptoms from two self-report measures of depression (the Beck Depression Inventory; BDI; Beck, Ward, Mendelson, Mock, & Erbaugh, 1961, and the Symptom Checklist-90; SCL-90; Derogatis, Lipman, & Covy, 1973) and two measures of anxiety (the Beck Anxiety Inventory; BAI; Beck, Epstein, Brown, & Steer, 1988, and the SCL-90) which were specifically chosen because they have been found to have good discriminant validity (Gotlib & Cane, 1989). Symptoms will also be compiled from scales which assess guilt, shame, dependency, self-criticism, interpersonal sensitivity, mastery, self-efficacy, self-esteem, hostility, and negative and positive affectivity (see below for a detailed description of these instruments). These constructs have all been depicted as important aspects of anxiety and/or depression. The symptoms and items will be the data which will be used in subsequent data analyses to determine whether there are unique symptoms 43 44 related only to depression versus unique symptoms related only to anxiety, and whether current instruments adequately measure those symptoms. A carefirl psychometric analysis of these symptoms and items will make it easier to begin to evaluate the instruments, as well as some of the theories which have been presented. It is possible that I will be able to differentiate groups of symptoms which are uniquely associated with either anxiety or depression. In that event, I will be able to evaluate whether the instruments commonly used to measure those constructs contain only relevant items, and also contain an adequate sampling of relevant items. It is also possible that I will be able to differentiate subgroups of anxiety (or depression) symptoms which relate to anxiety (or depression), but relate less well to each other, supporting the heterogeneity hypothesis. A third possibility is that I will be unable to distinguish meaningfirl clusters which distinguish symptoms of depression fiom those of anxiety. This would lend support to those theories which have conceptualized anxiety and depression as a continuum of mood disturbances. For example, Watson and Clark (1984) have suggested that we can better understand the commonalities and distinguishing features of depression and anxiety by looking at aspects of temperament, such as affective dispositions. They have argued for a model in which one higher order category, “negative affectivity,” is used to explain the high correlations between anxiety and depression, and the category “positive affectivity” is used to explain the imperfect correlation between those two syndromes. Other theorists (e. g. Tellegen, 1985) have argued that the general category of trait negative affectivity is 45 uniquely associated with anxiety, whereas it is low positive affectivity which uniquely characterizes depression. IO SO: Erb Self. then SeVe. Irma CHAPTER IV METHOD Participants Participants were 179 male and 187 female students between the ages of 17 and 29 (mean = 19) at a large midwestem university. Most of the students were Caucasian (83%), from middle to upper-middle class families. They were recruited from introductory psychology courses, and received credit toward course grades for participation. Instruments Demographic Information Demographic information was collected fi'om the students, including age, religion, social status, and ethnic background. Depression and Anxim The Beck Depression Inventory (BDI; Beck, Ward, Mendelson, Mock, & Erbaugh, 1961) was used to assess symptoms of depression. This scale consists of 84 self-evaluative statements which are grouped into 21 categories, Within each category, there are four item choices which increase in severity fi'om neutral ("1 do not feel sad") to severe ("I'm so sad or unhappy that I can't stand it"). Categories include mood, guilt, irritability, crying spells, and sleep and appetite disturbance. Items are scored fiom 0 to 3. 46 47 Scores of 24-63 reflect a severe level of depression; 16-23 suggest more moderate levels; 10-15 indicates a mild level of depression, and 0-9 indicates no depression (Shaw, Vallis, & McCabe, 1987). Although good split-half reliabilities have been reported (.86, on the average), factor analyses of the BDI with patients have resulted in three primary factors: sad mood/negative self-image, somatic complaints, and psychomotor retardation (Beck & Beamesderfer, 1974; Vredenburg, Krames, & Flett, 1985). Factor analysis of the current data was roughly consistent with this pattern, although researchers have not consistently found this pattern in nonclinical samples (Golin & Hartz, 1979; Lips & Ng, 1985). The BDI correlates well with both other self-report measures of depression, and clinician's ratings (Schwab, Bialow, & Holzer, 1967). The Beck Anxiety Inventory (BAI; Beck, Epstein, Brown, & Steer, 1988) was used to measure anxiety. The BA] is a 21 item scale derived from an item pool of anxiety symptoms. The items were selected to reflect a broad range of cognitive ("fear of dying"), somatic ("faint”; "feeling hot"), and affective (”scared"; "nervous") symptoms of anxiety, and to reduce overlap with symptoms of depression. The scale has shown good internal consistency (alpha = .92), and has been found to correlate more highly with other measures of anxiety (I = .51) than with measures of depression (1 = .25) in previous studies (Beck et al., 1988). The Symptom Checklist-90 (SCL-90; Derogatis, Lipman, & Covy, 1973) is a 90-item instrument designed to measure symptoms of psychological distress. The 48 individual is asked to rate on a 5-point scale (from 1 = M to 5 = xtremely) how much that problem has distressed them during the past 7 days. Five of the nine possible clusters were included in the inventory in this study: somatization ("headaches"; "faintness or dizziness"), depression ("blaming yourself for things"; "feeling no interest in things"), anxiety ("feeling tense or keyed up"; "the feeling that something bad is going to happen to you"), hostility ("feeling easily annoyed or irritated"; "having urges to beat, injure, or harm someone"), and interpersonal sensitivity ("your feelings being easily hurt"; "feeling very self-conscious with others"). Although some researchers have suggested that this instrument primarily measures one general dimension of psychopathology (Brophy, Norvell, & Kiluk, 1988; Cyr, McKenna-Foley, & Peacock, 1985), both statistically and substantively meaningful factors have been found (Shutty, DeGood, & Schwartz, 1986). This instrument has been found to have good internal consistency and test-retest reliability over short periods of time (Derogatis et al., 1973), and has been found to correlate strongly with other self-report instruments (Gotlib & Cane, 1989). PersonalfityDimenaipps The Depressive Experiences Questionnaire (DEQ; Blatt, D'Afilitti, & Quinlan, 1976a) was used to assess differences in depressive dimensions. This instrument does not assess symptoms of depression, but rather was designed to reflect differences in attitudes toward self and others believed to be relevant to depression. This inventory consists of 66 items which are rated on a 7-point scale (fi'om l = strongly disagree to 7 = trongly aggee). In previous studies, three primary factors have been identified and replicated: 49 Dependency, Self-Criticism, and Efficacy (Blatt, D'Afflitti, & Quinlan, 1976b). These factors also show strong test-retest stability (Zuroff, Moskowitz, Wielgus, Powers, & Franko, 1983). Construct validity was established in previous studies in which the dependent and self-critical scales predicted anaclitic and introjective depressive states, respectively Zuroff, Igreja, & Mongrain, 1990; Zuroff & Mongrain, 1987). For each of the three factors, those items fi'om the manual with factors loadings of over .40, which did not also load significantly on any of the other two factors, were selected for use in the current study. The Self-Criticism, or Introjective, scale consists of 12 items reflecting dissatisfaction with self, ambivalence toward others, and failure to meet standards or expectations ("there is a considerable difference between how I am now and how I would like to be"; "very frequently, my feelings toward someone close to me vary. There are times when I feel completely angry, and other times when I feel all-loving toward that person"). The Dependency, or Anaclitic, scale consists of 13 items which reflect fears of being rejected or hurting another person, in which pleasing others is very important and strong negative emotions are experienced as threatening to relationships ("I find it very difficult to say "No" to the requests of fiiends"; "if someone I cared about became angry with me, I would feel threatened that he (she) might leave me"). The Efficacy scale of the DEQ consists of 6 items which reflect achievement strivings and a sense of personal accomplishment ("I set my goals and standards as high as 50 possible"; "What I do and say has a very strong impact on those around me"). Affectivity was measured by the General Temperament Survey (GTS; Clark & Watson, 1989). The GTS consists of 3 scales which measure distinct aspects of personality. Items are rated as either true or false. The Negative Temperament scale consists of 28 items reflecting negative mood as well as self-concept. Watson and Clark (1993) report that individuals with high scores on this scale often experience sadness, anxiety, and guilt, as well as other negative affects ("I often feel nervous and ‘stressed’"; "I am often troubled by guilt feelings"; "I have days that I'm very irritable"). In addition, they are likely to be pessimistic and to have difficulties adjusting to failure or frustration ("I sometimes get too upset by minor setbacks"). In contrast, the Positive Temperament Scale measures the individual's tendency to experience positive affective states. This scale consists of 12 items reflecting positive affect ("I am usually enthusiastic about the things that I do"; "I often feel lively and cheerful for no particular reason"), 12 items indicative of high energy ("Most days I have a lot of "pep" or vigor"), and 3 additional, nonspecific, positive temperament items ("I am usually alert and attentive"; "I get excited when I think about the firture"; "I get pretty excited when I'm starting a new project"). Individuals who score highly on this scale tend to be cheerfiil and enthusiastic, and report more satisfying social interactions (Watson, 1988; Watson & Clark, in press). The Disinhibition versus Constraint Scale of the GTS consists of 35 items tapping traits thought to be relevant to the construct of disinhibition. Sample items include "I 51 rarely, if ever, do anything reckless" (scored negativelY); "I really enjoy beating the system"; and "taking care of details is not my strong point". In preliminary investigations, a longer version of this scale correlated highly with scales measuring Impulsivity (.68); Irresponsibility (.63); Risk Taking (.59); (Low) Persistence (.56); Playfulness (.54); Norm Rejection (.49); Danger Seeking (.47); and Disorganization (.40; Watson & Clark, 1993). Individuals who score highly on this scale are likely to be easygoing and somewhat irresponsible; to enjoy spontaneity, change, and excitement; and to have little motivation to conform to rules or to pursue traditional values. Preliminary investigations suggested that these three scales correlate well with instruments measuring similar constructs. In previous investigations, the Negative Temperament scale correlated .72 with the Negative Emotionality scale of the Multidimensional Personality Questionnaire (MPQ; Tellegen, in press), and .84 with the Neuroticism scale of the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire (EPQ; Eysenck & Eysenck, 1975; (Watson & Clark, 1993; Watson & Clark, in press). Positive Temperament correlated .76 with the Positive Emotionality scale of the MPQ, and .68'with the Neuroticism scale of the EPQ. The preliminary measure of disinhibition correlated -.56 with the Constraint scale of the MPQ, .55 with the Psychoticism scale of the EPQ, and -.61 with the Conscientiousness scale of the NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI; Costa & McCrae, 1985) (Watson & Clark, 1992). 52 Affective Self-Evaluations Measures of self-evaluations, which have been enumerated as symptoms of depression were included in an Affect Inventory composed of items from four scales. Self-esteem was measured with the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (Rosenberg, 1965). Items are endorsed on a 5-point scale from strongly disagree (1) to strongly agree (5), and include "on the whole, I am satisfied with myself". Mastery was assessed using the Mastery Scale from the Offer Self-Image Questionnaire (Offer, Ostrov, & Howard, 1989). This scale consists of 10 items which are endorsed on a 5-point scale from strongly disagree (1) to strongly agree (5), and include "IfI put my mind to it I can learn almost anything". This scale has shown adequate concurrent validity (Ostrov et al., 1989). Guilt was measured by the Chang and Hunter Guilt Scale (Chang, 1988). This scale consists of 9 items based on a definition of guilt in terms of perceptions of self as causing harm to others and efforts toward making reparations when harm has been done. Items include "Sometimes I cannot forgive myself for having caused deep pain in those I love or care for". Items are rated on a 5-point scale from strongly disagree (1) to sgo_ngly m (5). Global shame was measured by items fiom the Intemalized Shame Scale (ISS: Cook, 1985) as selected by Chang and Hunter (Chang, 1988). The revised Shame Scale consists of the 11 items found to measure shame (e. g., "I feel like I am never quite good enough"), eliminating those which Chang found to address other constructs. The items 53 are negatively worded, and are rated on a 5-point scale ranging from myer (1) to a1n_ro_§r am (5). Procedure The self-report measures described previously were administered to the students in a group setting. Participants were informed that the purpose of the study was to better understand the affective experiences of college students. They were also informed as to their rights as subjects. To ensure anonymity, data were identified only by code numbers. Participants received extra course credit for their participation. lv—d CHAPTER V RESULTS Hierarchical Structure of the Data Analysis The analysis of the data for this dissertation is hierarchical in nature. Since hierarchical analysis is unfamiliar to many readers, this first section of the results section will outline the steps in the hierarchical analysis. The specific steps in the actual analysis will then be presented in detail. Terminology The items selected were those which various authors have used to define "anxiety and "depression". These items can be considered using two different psychological terminologies. The psychiatric orientation focuses on a short term way of thinking, perhaps best captured by the word "symptom". Symptoms are assumed to vary considerably over time depending on environmental stress as well as the underlying status of the individual; the latter may be especially variable in the case of people diagnosed with bipolar disorders. Much of the present data is discussed in these terms. The second terminology being utilized may best be conceptualized using the language of trait theory. For example, most psychologists would expect shame and self- esteem to be long-term personality traits showing only slight variation over short durations, such as a week or a month. For such items, the natural terminology would be 54 tem item 8an dysl Ofie the SCC( mes «Van depr 55 terms such as "characteristic" or "trait". The terminology used here will vary from one item to another depending on the typical usage pattern in clinical psychology, though at this point there is no attempt to sort the items in terms of etiology or assumed time variation. The Hierarchical Structure of Symptoms The single items collected in this study are directed at specific experiences or specific facts. The concept of " symptom" is a higher order concept. A symptom such as "feelings of dependency" could be expressed in a variety of experiences. Thus, we would predict that items would form clusters in which the items within a given cluster all measure the same symptom as expressed by different Specific experiences. The concepts of "anxiety" and "depression" are higher order concepts. For example, depression is thought to be expressed in various symptoms such as anhedonia or dysphoria. There is no established language for this higher level, although the word most often used is "syndrome". In this way, items can be grouped in a hierarchical pattern. At the first level, items can be clustered in terms of the symptoms they measure. At the second level, symptoms can be clustered in terms of the syndrome that is expressed. Some authors (cf. Watson & Clark, 1992; Watson, Clark, & Carey, 1988) have argued for a still higher order factor. In the case of the symptoms considered in this thesis, the relevant highest order concept is that of "neuroticism" or "negative affectivity". Many current authors (cf. Clark, Watson, & Mineka, 1994) consider both anxiety and depression to be expressions of this higher order trait, along with loneliness, shame, self- 56 consciousness, and other negative affects. Some authors include hostility in this higher order trait. The firll hierarchical clustering scheme could be considered as a nested classification of the items at the following levels: Level 1 - items are clustered by symptom Level 2 - symptoms are clustered by syndrome Level 3 - syndromes are clustered by major personality factor. In this thesis I conducted data analyses at all of these levels. Items and Symptoms or Characteristics I began with 290 items. However, there was never any consideration of the possibility that there are 290 different symptoms or characteristics to be measured. Rather, it was assumed that items could be clustered into sets in which the items within a set measure the same symptom or characteristic. For simplicity, this section will be written in the language of symptoms, even though characteristics will be also be considered in the analysis. At the first level of analysis, the items were clustered into symptoms. This was initially done using content analysis. The clusters were then evaluated using confirmatory factor analysis. A major problem revealed by the confirmatory factor analysis was with items using a binary response format. Items using a binary response format suffer from a response set problem that was not anticipated when the study was designed; a key article detailing this problem by Green, Goldman, and Salovey (1993) had not been published until prese him the er of net discar 39 ch chara< measu the nu items ' the lie the lon items C II is lhl SWPIO cOnsisn the Squz 57 until after the data were gathered. This issue will be discussed in detail following the presentation of the content analysis. However, due to the problems encountered with the binary items, those items were dropped from the analysis. That is, in spite of regrets over the content lost thereby, the items from the Watson and Clark inventory measuring aspects of negative affectivity, positive affectivity, and disinhibition versus constraint were discarded from this analysis. Without the Watson and Clark items, there are 200 items that were classified into 29 clusters. That is, the items under analysis cover 29 different symptoms or characteristics identified by the content analysis. On average there are about 4 items measuring each symptom or characteristic. However, there is a great deal of variation in the number of items written to cover each symptom or characteristic. Once the binary items were removed, the confirmatory factor analysis showed good fit for the clustering of the items. Consider the items in a cluster measuring one symptom, such as "fear" (although the longer word "apprehension" would fit the content more closely). With the binary items discarded, there were 11 items identified in the content analysis as measuring fear. It is thus possible to use these items to define a fear scale. This scale would measure the symptom of fear, although that measurement would be imperfect. The internal consistency of the fear scale was .89, so the measurement is quite good: a correlation of the square root of .89 (.94) between the scale score for fear and the fear construct itself. Authors differ in the extent to which they have considered various symptoms in defining slight c exampl Hyper; items 2 tepres: is only reliabi; COITel.‘ Corral; Scales 537nm be let the co adlUSt: Clues, 58 defining the constructs of anxiety and depression. Some symptoms have been given only slight consideration by any authors. This was very much evident in the present data. For example, the cluster measuring Psychomotor Tension had 10 items and Autonomic Hyperarousal had 17 items; however, there were many symptoms represented by only two items and some symptoms were represented by only single items. The symptoms represented by the latter items were regarded as poorly measured. Symptoms: ScaLesimd Constructs The actual symptom of fear is the construct measured by the Fear scale. The scale is only an imperfect measurement of that symptom. If more fear items were used, the reliability would go up and the scale would be a better measure of the symptom. Using confirmatory factor analysis, it is possible to obtain estimates of the correlations between symptoms rather than the correlations between scales. These correlations are corrected for the attenuation produced by error of measurement in the scales (see Appendix B). The problem with the estimated correlations between constructs is that many symptoms were measured by only one or two items. The reliability of such scales tend to be very low and thus correction for attenuation requires a large adjustment in the size of the correlation. This greatly exaggerates the problem with sampling error. A large adjustment greatly increases the sampling error in the estimated correlation. In some cases, the sampling error can cause an estimated correlation to be larger than 1.00: an outcome which is primarily troublesome for those not well trained in reliability theory. A popula' error 0 Thus, 1 interpr "depre S}ndt< Slmpt depres been: 59 population correlation Cannot be larger than 1.00, but a sample correlation corrected for error of measurement can be larger than 1 because of sampling in the estimation process. Thus, for those symptoms measured by very few items, the corrected correlations must be interpreted with great care. Smptoms and Syndromes The key questions in the controversies over the definition of "anxiety" and "depression" can be stated at the next level of the hierarchical analysis; are there separate syndromes for anxiety and depression? If so, which symptoms define each syndrome? The key statistical analysis for this question starts with the correlations between symptoms. Are there two clusters of symptoms that can be identified as anxiety and depression, respectively? If so, which items belong to each cluster? One method by which such questions have been considered in the literature has been exploratory factor analysis: sometimes referred to simply as "factor analysis". Advocates for exploratory factor analysis claim that this method will identify the underlying factors and the corresponding clusters of variables that measure each factor. Exploratory factor analysis was done for the symptom correlations: one analysis of the symptom construct correlations (i.e., corrected for attenuation) and another analysis of the symptom scale correlations (i.e., not corrected for attenuation). There are two fundamental problems with exploratory factor analysis: (a) it assumes uncorrelated basic traits and (b) it ignores causal relations between variables. If these two conditions are not met, then exploratory factor analysis may give very nine theo confi nlcu case. phyn nnkl sufltr Create Inathe Shdn been: nil b. \3 cOnsid chfllac plOble 6O misleading results. For highly correlated constructs that are causally related to each other, the only form of factor analysis that gives good estimates of factor correlations is confirmatory factor analysis. Consider anxiety and depression: By whatever form of measurement, the literature has consistently shown these two syndromes to be very highly correlated. Furthermore, all current theories predict that they should be highly correlated (or "co-morbid" in the case of extreme cases). High anxiety should cause depression because of the neural physiology of the anxiety process. High anxiety causes a depletion of norepinephrine which results in state depression (Weiss & Simson, 1985). On the other hand, people who suffer depression also suffer many impairments of social and work life. This in turn would create stress and accompanying anxiety. In this way, current theory regarding anxiety and depression predicts that the mathematical factors of exploratory factor analysis will be far removed from the actual syndromes that cause the symptoms. For this reason, the exploratory factor analysis will be considered only as a hypothesis generating device. The real test of the syndrome model will be made using confirmatory factor analysis. The Structure of the Results Section The results themselves will be developed in segments. The first segment will consider the process of clustering items to form scales that measure symptoms and characteristics. This section will present the content analysis and briefly discuss the problems caused by the binary item format used in the Watson and Clark inventory. The keylnu and otl to beu snnptc that an. lnaddl conela higher 1 'pure" ”mixed defined Slndror 75 ben morbidj cOmelat c(”Bidet all-em” 61 key outcome from this segment is that the 200 non-binary items measure 29 symptoms and other characteristics. The correlations between these symptoms are the basic results to be used to test the various syndrome hypotheses for anxiety and depression. The second segment presents the results of the analysis of the correlations between symptoms. This will include a discussion of the exploratory factor analysis, even though that analysis was flawed by the high correlations between the basic constructs measured. In addition to the high correlation between anxiety and depression, there were also high correlations between those syndromes and other syndromes and characteristics. The confirmatory factor analysis shows that the 29 symptoms do indeed define 10 higher order syndromes and traits. Two of these syndromes are tentatively defined as "pure" measures of anxiety and depression while two other syndromes are defined as "mixed" measures of anxiety and depression. There are 6 other syndromes or traits defined by the 29 basic symptoms and characteristics. The third segment of results will start with the correlations between the 10 syndromes identified by clusters of symptoms. A key fact is the very high correlation of .75 between pure anxiety and pure depression, which matches the high level of co- morbidity found in epidemiological studies. Various causal interpretations of these correlations can be considered. Some authors have argued that all of the syndromes defined in this thesis can be considered as expressions of one underlying trait called "neuroticism" or "negative afi‘ectivity". This hypothesis will be considered and a factor analysis of the 10 syndromes nil OK pC an I? In Ct: 62 will be considered in this regard. Much important information is lost if only the higher order factor is considered and the individual syndromes are ignored. Itemnd Symptoms This segment presents l) reliabilities of the original scales fi'om which the item pool was derived and 2) an analysis of the 290 items into clusters that measure symptoms and other characteristics. Reliabilities Depression and anxieg. Internal consistency for the BDI was .90 in the current sample. In the current study, the BDI correlated highly with the SCL-90 depression scale (.72), yet also correlated highly with anxiety scales (.65; .67). This is consistent with other researchers who have found poor discriminant validity between the EDI and measures of anxiety (Mullaney, 1987). The reliability coefficient for the BAI in the current sample was .93. The BAI correlated highly with the other measure of anxiety (_r_ = .82), but did not shOw good discrimination fi'om the measures of depression (rs = .65; .74). Internal consistency for the scales of the SCL-90 in the current study were .88 (somatization), .87 (anxiety), .91 (depression), .79 (hostility), and .86 (interpersonal sensitivity). Personalig dimensions. Internal consistency findings for the DEQ were .82 for the Introjective scale, .73 for the Anaclitic scale, and .75 for the Efficacy scale. Internal consistency findings for the GTS were .88 for the Negative Temperament scale, .85 for the Positive Temperament, and .79 for the Disinhibition scale. 63 Affective self-evaluations. Internal consistency for the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale was .86 for the present sample. Internal consistency for the Mastery Scale was .71. Internal consistency for the Chang and Hunter Guilt Scale was .77. Internal consistency for the revised Shame Scale was .92. Items and Smptoms The first step was a content analysis of all items included in the study (See Appendix A). This was followed by a confirmatory factor analysis to test the content model (See Appendix B). The confirmatory factor analysis showed three sets of problems. The most severe problem was caused by the binary response format for the General Temperament Survey (GTS) Inventory. These data showed the same response set problem reported by Green, Goldman, and Salovey (1993), who showed how the use of forced-choice format can distort results because of systematic response bias. As a result, several clusters were split into binary and non-binary clusters. For example, an initial "won'y" cluster was split between "worry" and "worry-binary". There are ways to statistically reduce the effect of the binary response set, but the GTS inventory does not have the right mix of items to use those methods. So ultimately all clusters measured using the binary format were dropped. The confirmatory factor analysis also showed problems for two other original clusters. The SCL-90 cluster for "interpersonal sensitivity" was not questioned in the original content analysis. However, the confirmatory factor analysis showed that it is multidimensional and needed further analysis. This ultimately produced two clusters QB. dl 31‘. fat fat 64 called "Unliked" and "Self-Conscious." The confirmatory factor analysis also showed problems for the "Worry" cluster. A Priori Content Analysis The initial content analysis focused on symptoms associated with anxiety and depression. Exemplars were taken from manuals of clinical diagnostic criteria, such as research diagnostic criteria, and theoretical and empirical papers, with special attention given to the DSM-II-R criteria to ensure that the symptoms under study reflected the dimensions which have been depicted as important in diagnosing these disorders. Special attention was also given to those theorists who have delineated explicit theories about factors associated with depression and/or anxiety, and developed instruments using those factors, such as Beck. These sources were chosen, as mentioned previously, because the instruments in question have been found to have good discriminant validity (Gotlib & Cane, 1989). An attempt was made to sample the relevant literature broadly, across theoretical perspectives, for symptoms which would be accessible through self-report. The symptoms, or themes, were then divided into 3 groups: those associated with depression only, those associated with anxiety only, and those associated with both depression and anxiety. The symptoms most specifically associated with depression included dysphoria, low self-esteem, indecisiveness, hopelessness, psychomotor , retardation, anhedonia, feelings of guilt or worthlessness, and suicidal ideation. Those symptoms most specifically linked to anxiety included feelings of worry, anxiety, disquietude, and irritability; vigilance; fears of dying or being out of control; somatic 65 symptoms, such as shortness of breath, dizziness, chest pains, aches, flushes, numbness, and an exaggerated startle response; depersonalization or derealization; and shyness. Symptoms which were linked to both anxiety and depression included appetite disturbance and abdominal distress, sleep disturbance, fatiguability, concentration difficulties, and agitation. A content analysis of the items from all scales was performed, which yielded the following themes: Fear, Motor Tension, Autonomic Hyperarousal, Vigilance, Irritability, Insecure, Upset, Tense, Worry, Dysphoric Mood, Fatigue, Energy, Enthusiasm, Sleep Disturbance, Appetite Disturbance, Cognitive Disturbance, Dependency, Insufficiency, Need for Approval, Self-Critical, Anhedonia, Self-Esteem, Efficacy, Unstable, Guilt, Anger, Hostility, Negative Afi‘ectivity, Positive Affectivity, Hopelessness, Suicidality, Interpersonal Sensitivity, Impulsivity, Irresponsible, Playful, Antisocial, Sensation Seeking, Disorganized, and Ambitious. Also included in the analysis were the clusters formed by the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (SE), the Mastery Scale (Mastery), the Cook Shame Scale (Shame), and the Chang-Hunter Guilt Scale, respectively. Each of these scales was homogeneous enough to be included in the analysis as complete clusters. A complete list of items in each cluster is included as Appendix A. The “Fear” cluster consisted of five items from the Beck Anxiety Inventory (BAI), five items from the Anxiety subscale of the Symptom Checklist (SCL—90), and one item from the Depression subscale of the SCL-90: “feelings of being caught or trapped.” The items within this cluster depicted general feelings of fearfirlness and apprehension, such as ln' ch; reh EX; gene 66 “the thought that something bad is going to happen to you,” as well as specific fears, such as “fear of dying” and “fear of losing control.” The “Motor Tension” cluster consisted of four items from the Somatization subscale of the SCL-90, three items from the Anxiety subscale of the SCL—90, and three items from the BAI. The items depicted many of the types of motor tension symptoms often associated with anxiety, such as aches, pains, shakiness, and trembling. The third cluster, “Autonomic Hyperarousal,” consisted of 11 items from the BAI, five items fi'om the Somatization subscale of the SCL-90, and one item from the Anxiety subscale of the SCL-90. It consisted of items associated with autonomic hyperarousal, such as dizziness, abdominal discomfort, heart pounding or racing, and hot or cold spells. Cluster 4 was defined by symptoms of “Vigilance,” often associated with anxiety disorders. It was composed of two items from the BA]: “unable to relax” and “nervous,” and one item fi'om the Anxiety subscale of the SCL-90: “feeling tense or keyed up.” Cluster 5, “Irritability,” was composed of one item from the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI), and one item fiom the Hostility subscale of the SCL-90, which were characterized by feelings of extreme or excessive irritability. The sixth cluster was characterized by “Insecurity,” especially as regards relationships, and consisted of two items from the Introjective subscale of the Depressive Experiences Questionnaire (DEQ). Clusters 7 through 9 consisted of items describing feelings often associated with general distress, or “negative affectivity.” Cluster 7 was termed “Upset,” and consisted of 67 four items from the NA subscale of the GTS, which described a tendency to become overly upset at small setbacks. Cluster 8 was termed “Tense.” It consisted of three items from the NA subscale of the GTS which described a tendency to feel tense, stressed, or “on edge.” The ninth cluster was termed “Worry.” It consisted of one item fiom the BDI: “I am so worried about my physical problems that I cannot think of anything else,” and one item from the Depression subscale of the SCL—90: “worrying too much about things.” Clusters 10 and 11 described symptoms often associated with depression. Cluster 10, termed “Dysphoric Mood” consisted of 3 items fi'om the Depression subscale of the SCL-90 and 2 items from the BDI. These items described many of the feelings often associated with depression, such as loneliness, sadness, and tearfulness. Cluster 11, on the other hand, consisted of symptoms of the “Fatigue” which is often associated with depression. This cluster consisted of two items from the Depression subscale of the SCL- 90, two items from the BDI, and one item from the NA subscale of the GTS. The items depicted lethargy, tiredness, and the feeling that life “feels like a big struggle.” Clusters l2 and 13 consisted of items from subscales of the Positive Temperament (PA) subscale of the GTS. Cluster 12, “Energy,” consisted of all 12 items of the “Energy” subscale, and described an active, energetic, fast-paced lifestyle. Cluster 13, “Enthusiasm,” consisted of three items from the Positive Temperament subscale and two items from the general Positive Affect subscale. The items depicted an enthusiastic and excited attitude toward life. Clusters 14 through 16 described some of the disturbances often associated with 68 depression. Cluster 14, “Sleep Disturbance,” consisted of one item from the EDI and one item from the NA subscale of the GTS, describing sleep difficulties most often associated with endogenous, or more severe, depression. Cluster 15, “Appetite Disturbance,” consisted of two items from the BDI describing lack of appetite and extreme weight loss. Cluster 16, “Cognitive Difficulties,” consisted of two items from the NA subscale of the GTS describing mental confusion and troubling thoughts or ideas. Clusters 17 through 19 each consisted of items from the DEQ which focus on interpersonal needs. Cluster 17, “Dependency,” consisted of eight items from the Anaclitic subscale of the DEQ, describing intense needs for interpersonal relatedness and difficulties in being alone. Cluster 18, “Insufliciency,” consisted of two items from the Introjective subscale of the DEQ which describe feelings of helplessness and emptiness. Cluster 19, “Need for Approval,” consisted of five items from the Anaclitic subscale of the DEQ which are characterized by a focus on attempting to please others as well as fears of being criticized by or offending others. Cluster 20, “Self-Critical,” consisted of three items from the EDI and one item from the Introjective subscale of the DEQ which are characterized by self-blame, self- hatred, and feeling of failure. Cluster 21, “Anhedonia,” consisted of three items from the BDI and two items from the Depression subscale of the SCL-90, and is characterized by feelings of disinterest, boredom, and a lack of positive investment in living. Clusters 22 and 23 focused on feelings of self-esteem and self-efficacy. Cluster 22, 69 “Self-Esteem,” consisted of three items from the Self-Efficacy subscale of the DEQ, one item from the Interpersonal Sensitivity subscale of the SCL-90, and one item from the Depression subscale of the SCL-90. These items address a sense of inner strength and worth. Cluster 23, “Efficacy,” consisted of three items from the Self-Efficacy subscale of the DEQ, which describe high goals, standards, and expectations. Cluster 24, “Unstable,” consisted of two items from the Introjective subscale of the DEQ, which describe extreme variability in feelings toward self and others. Clusters 25 through 28 each was characterized by negative affect. Cluster 25, “Guilt,” consisted of one item from the EDI, and one item from the NA subscale of the GTS which described feelings of guilt. Cluster 26, “Anger,” consisted of three items from the NA subscale of the GTS describing fi'equent, uncontained, or irrational anger. Cluster 27, “Hostility,” consisted of five items fiom the Hostility subscale of the SCL-90 and one item from the Interpersonal Sensitivity subscale of the SCL—90 and consisted of items describing feeling critical toward others, temper outbursts, and aggressive urges. Cluster 28, “Negative Affectivity,” consisted of two items from the NA subscale of the GTS which describe general negative affectivity in terms to vague to be included in those clusters which describe more specific negative feelings. Cluster 29, “Positive Affectivity,” consisted of six items from the Positive Affect subscale of the GTS, which characterize a positive, active investment in life and living. Clusters 30 and 31 depicted some of the more severe symptoms associated with depression. Cluster 30, “Hopelessness,” consisted of one item from the BDI and one item Lo) m0: the ( 70 from the Depression subscale of the SCL-9O describing feelings of hopelessness. Cluster 3], “Suicidality,” consisted of one item from the BDI and one item from the Depression subscale of the SCL-9O describing the desire to end one’s life. Cluster 32, “Interpersonal Sensitivity,” consisted of eight items from the Interpersonal Sensitivity subscale of the SCL-90, describing feelings of self-consciousness and uneasiness with others, as well as fears of being disliked or regarded as inferior. Clusters 33, 34, and 35 consisted of items from the Disinhibition scale of the GTS. Cluster 33, “Impulsivity,” consisted of eight items which are characterized by a somewhat reckless, incautious, and unreasoned stance toward life. Cluster 34, “Irresponsibility,” consisted of one item which is reverse scored: “1 am a serious-minded person.” Cluster 35, “Persistence,” consisted of one item which is reverse scored: “1 work just hard enough to get by.” This item was also included in Cluster 40. Cluster 36, “Playful,” consisted of three items fiom the Positive Affect subscale of the GTS which are characterized by feelings of enthusiasm and playfulness. Clusters 37 through 40 consisted largely of items from the Disinhibition subscale of the GTS. Cluster 37, “Antisocial,” consisted of 11 items characterized by a lack of regard for accepted rules and standards for social behavior, as well as a willingness to hurt others to obtain a desired goal. Cluster 38, “Sensation Seeking,” consisted of eight Disinhibition items, as well as one Positive Affect item, which are characterized by a desire for excitement, novelty, and thrills. Cluster 39, “Disorganization,” consisted of one item: “taking care of details is not my strong point.” Cluster 40, “Ambitious,” consisted of two 71 items which describe a willingness to work hard to achieve desired goals. Clusters 41 through 44 consisted of the “Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale,” the “Mastery Scale,” the items from the “Cook Shame Scale,” and the “Chang-Hunter Guilt Scale,” respectively. These scales have been described in detail in the Method section. Confirmatogg Factor Analysis of Items A confirmatory factor analysis tested empirically the item clusters identified by the content analysis. The results of the confirmatory factor analysis are considered cluster by cluster. For each cluster, it was possible to check to see if the items in that cluster form a coherent or "unidimensional" set. In order for a set of items to be unidimensional, they must all measure the same construct. For this data, that means that all items must measure the same symptom or the same trait. This is tested in confirmatory factor analysis by examining each cluster for internal consistency - the pattern of correlations among items in the same cluster - and for parallelism - the pattern of correlations between the items in that cluster and the other symptoms and characteristics measured. The confirmatory factor analysis also computes the alpha or Spearman-Brown reliability of each cluster. Binagg format response set. The main clusters manifesting severe problems were clusters with items fi'om the General Temperament Survey (GTS) which uses a binary response format. The binary items were not parallel to the other items. The binary items showed much stronger correlations with all GTS clusters than did the non-binary items. 72 The binary items across different clusters such as "Irritability" or "Fear" correlated much more highly with binary items from the other clusters than did the other, non-binary, items. This pattern of correlation for binary response format items has been found in other studies (as reviewed in Green, Goldman, & Salovey, 1993). The fact that this pattern derives from a binary format response set was proven in data gathered for that purpose by Green and his colleagues (1993). Had that study been published when the data for this study was collected, the design would have been changed and a non-binary format would have been used. There have been some heuristic methods used in previous studies to reduce the effects of the binary response set, but because of the specific structure of the GTS, these methods could not be used in this study. The net results were these: First, each cluster defined by both binary and non-binary items was split to form two clusters; one with binary and one with non-binary items. Second, for the main purposes of the study, all binary item clusters were dropped from fithher consideration. Interpersonal sensitivity. The SCL-9O has a scale of items devoted explicitly to "interpersonal sensitivity". The confirmatory factor analysis showed that this scale is n_ot unidimensional. A detailed content analysis of the “Interpersonal Sensitivity” items was then done. This identified two specific clusters of items which were labeled "Unliked" and "Self-Conscious". The "Unliked" cluster consisted of three items stating that the person 73 feels unliked by others. The "Self-Conscious" cluster consisted of three items asking if the person often feels self-conscious. There were three other items in the "Interpersonal Sensitivity" scale. The item that read "feeling critical of others" was added to the Hostility cluster. The other two items were just dropped (i.e., put in the "Residual" cluster). Wm The "Worry" cluster is defined by only two non-binary items. These items initially seemed similar in content, but the empirical pattern of correlations was dissimilar for the two items. When correlations between the "worry" scale and other scales are corrected for attenuation, many of the estimated correlations are larger than 1.00. So the initial "Worry" cluster does not work. The two Worry items are : 149. SCL-DEP Worrying too much about things. 128. BDI I am so worried about my physical problems that I cannot think about anything else. The problem seems to be with the BDI item. It is specific to physical problems, whereas many people are more worried about social or personal problems. Furthermore, there is an element of obsessiveness in the item that is inconsistent for an item written as a depression item rather than as an anxiety item. This cluster was dropped from further analysis. The Final Item Clusters The final confirmatory factor analysis began with 49 item clusters. The items for each cluster are listed in Appendix A. The binary format GTS items are those with 74 numbers 201 to 290. In cases where a content cluster would have included items from the GTS and items from the non-binary scales, that cluster was split so that the binary items were put in a separate cluster. Because there seems to be no way to solve the binary response set problem in these data, all clusters using the binary format were reluctantly dropped from further analysis. Clusters 45 through 48 consisted of NA items which were not included in the previous clusters of the same names, because of differences in response set between the Likert and binary formats. Cluster 45, “Irritability (GTS),” consisted of two NA items describing extreme irritability. Cluster 46, “Worry (GTS),” consisted of three NA items which describe frequent or excessive worrying. Cluster 47, “Fear (GTS),” consisted of two NA items characterized by fearfirl apprehension. Finally, Cluster 48, “Sleep Disturbance (GTS),” consisted of one NA item: “I often have difficulty sleeping because of my worries.” Cluster 49, “Alienated,” consisted of three items from the Interpersonal Sensitivity scale of the SCL-90, characterized by feeling easily hurt and that others are unfiiendly or unsympathetic. The 29 symptoms retained for firrther analysis are listed and described in Table 9. The correlations among symptoms can be computed either using the items as scales or using confirmatory factor analysis to produce construct correlations. The confirmatory factor analysis construct correlations are the same as the scale correlations corrected for attenuation due to random error of measurement: i.e., the same as the scale 75 Table 9 Number of Items and Reliabilities for Symptoms Retained for Further Analysis SYMPTOM NUMBER OF ITEMS RELIABILITY" Fear 1 1 89 Motor Tension 10 85 Autonomic Hyperarousal 17 92 Vigilance 3 77 Irritability 2 51 Insecure 2 47 Worry 2 35 Dysphoric Mood 5 80 Fatigue 4 73 Sleep 1 100 Appetite 2 34 Dependency 8 65 Insufficiency 2 59 Need for Approval 5 48 Self-Critical 6 73 Anhedonia 6 76 Self-Esteem 5 64 SelfiEflicacy 3 56 Unstable 2 60 Guilt 2 50 Hostility 6 80 Hopelessness 2 67 Suicidality 2 73 Self-Conscious 3 72 76 Table 9 (cont’d). SYMPTOM NUMBER OF ITEMS RELIABILITY Rosenberg Self-Esteem 10 85 Mastery 10 71 Shame 10 91 Chang-Hunter Guilt 9 76 Alienated 3 76 "' All decimals omitted. correlations corrected using the alpha reliabilities. The symptom correlation matrix will be presented later since it is easier to consider after the symptoms have been arranged by syndrome. Smptoms and Synfdromes The next step in the analysis was to see how the symptoms related to each other. The key question is this: Are there two syndromes corresponding to anxiety and depression? The second key question is this: Are the other characteristics claimed to be measures of depression really the same as depression or are they separate syndromes or traits in their own right? An earlier generation of psychologists was dominated by psychometricians who believed that the dimensionality of a set of variables could be determined by a method now 77 called "exploratory factor analysis". However in recent years, most psychometricians have joined earlier critics and come to the belief that dimensionality is better determined by a method that is now called "confirmatory factor analysis". Both exploratory and confirmatory factor analysis were applied to the symptom correlations to determine the number and identity of syndromes. The exploratory factor analysis fit the name "exploratory"; it suggested rough syndrome categories but those categories had to be revised to meet the more stringent criteria set by confirmatory factor analysis. The following segment will describe the steps taken to form the final syndromes. The results of the final syndrome analysis will then be presented. Exploratogz factor analysis Exploratory factor analysis is often called just "factor analysis" in many articles, especially older articles. By far the common method of exploratory factor analysis is a two step process: (a) a principal axis factor analysis done with communalities, (b) followed by VARIMAX rotation. The beginning symptom correlation matrix can take one of two forms: (a) the correlations among symptom scales or ”(b) the correlations among symptom constructs. Both analyses were done and proved to have similar results. The exploratory factor analysis of the symptom construct correlation matrix is presented in Table 10. Two factors identified symptom clusters that were very similar in content. Two factors identify clusters with only slight modifications. Two factors identify specific [Mr \ .hru \ mpr\ \ k S 9|. HI. a . .\ «a. r... \ . . .11 Mn . . Y .r ARK l a 1 VI“ The Exploratory Factor Analysis of the Symptom Construct Correlation Matrix. 78 Table 10 SYMPTOM l 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 Autonomic Hyperarousal 85* ~8 37 15 8 ~1 18 4 ~8 6 Motor Tension 84" ~18 35 26 14 10 14 3 ~5 1 Fear 66"I ~27 45 32 20 18 l ~7 7 ~7 Vigilance 65* ~15 13 46 22 25 12 ~6 l6 ~1 Hostility 62" ~23 38 30 12 ~3 14 21 ~6 16 Self-Esteem (Rosenberg) ~l 1 90" ~26 ~5 ~22 1 ~12 1 ~7 ~5 Mastery ~16 90" ~22 ~13 ~25 ~7 ~5 ~7 l ~l Self-Esteem ~30 72* ~32 ~30 ~12 2O ~9 6 38 ~1 Shame 19 ~70* 17 28 33 16 9 20 ~3 0 Self-Critical 21 ~54* 52 35 34 l 2 12 7 ~9 3 Guilt 31 ~27 89* 16 12 2 10 16 ~2 6 Appetite Disturbance 42 ~26 85" 15 ~12 ~7 33 ~2 ~13 10 Suicidality 40 ~28 75* 21 3 ~9 -5 2 -1 ~25 Anhedonia 35 ~26 64* 39 20 ~6 16 2 5 24 Hopelessness 29 ~41 54* 44 30 10 15 ~18 8 12 Alienated 47 ~18 25 74* 19 22 8 8 ~7 ~9 Dysphoric Mood 42 ~18 40 67* 27 10 l 1 ~14 ~2 ~1 Initability 41 ~25 39 66“ 30 13 l3 l6 ~2 19 Fatigue 43 ~22 36 55* 22 10 2O 1 0 42 Self-Conscious 47 ~29 1 5 50" 20 3O 22 l 8 ~16 ~7 ' Identifies symptom’s highest positive or negative correlation. All decimals omitted. 79 Table 10 (cont’d). SYMPTOMS I 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 Unstable 9 ~20 ~2 21 91 " 24 6 1 3 8 ~4 Insufliciency 30 ~33 1 l 25 76* 33 9 4 ~S 5 Insecure 14 ~44 23 12 74‘I 32 4 -7 ~6 10 Need for Approval 9 ~3 ~5 14 23 92* O 14 5 ~7 Dependency 11 ~18 18 18 41 78* 4 ~12 ~19 14 Efiicacy ~2 45 ~3 ~3 l 1 62* ~7 3 41 1 Sleep Disturbance 24 ~14 15 15 8 ~l 67* ~1 ~1 3 Guilt (Chang-Hunter) 13 ~27 20 5 5 33 ~6 41"I 3 0 ‘ Identifies syrnptom’s highest positive or negative correlation. All decimals omitted. symptoms that seem to have no clear syndromal meaning. Two factors have no large correlates. However, there are two factors where the statistical clusters are not well defined by considering only the highest loadings. The discussion below considers the simplest cases first. No symptoms had their highest correlations with Factors 9 or 10. On the other hand, the factor analysis for 10 factors is clearer than the analysis for 8 factors. In this case, these "extra" factors seem to reduce the impact of sampling error. Factor 8 has a modestly high correlation only with the trait Guilt construct. Trait 80 Guilt does not show a pattern in common with any other symptom. Factor 7 has a modestly high correlation only with Sleep Disturbance. This symptom has weak correlations with other factors. Factor 2 has very high correlations with the five symptoms related to Shame or Self-Esteem. These symptoms are used to define a syndrome of Shame (to focus on the negative end). Factor 5 has very high correlations with the three symptoms related to feelings of Insecurity. These symptoms are used to define a syndrome of Insecurity. Factor 6 has very high correlations with the symptoms that tap feelings of Need for Others (i.e., Need for Approval and Dependency). Feelings of self-efficacy also correlate highly with Factor 6, but Efficacy clearly differs from the two measures of Need for Others in that Efficacy has a high correlation of +.45 with the Self-Esteem factor (Factor 2) while the Alienation measures correlate -.03 and ~.18. Thus, only the two symptoms that tap Need for Others are used to define a syndrome. Factor 1 has very high correlations with the symptoms most closely identified with anxiety: i.e., Autonomic Hyperarousal, Motor Tension, Fear (or apprehension), and Vigilance. Hostility also correlates highly with Factor 1, but is clearly different in content. The four conventional symptoms are used to define a syndrome of Anxiety. The symptom of Hostility is so important that it is used by itself to define a syndrome. Factors 3 and 4 are complicated. Both factors have very high correlations with symptoms associated with depression. Examination of the firll set of factor loadings 81 shows that neither factor lines up with clearly delineated clusters. This seems to represent a cases where exploratory factor analysis rotates to a misleading position. The two symptoms that tap feelings of Alienation (feeling Alienated and Self~ Conscious) both have high correlations with Factor 4, though they line up at opposite ends. On the other hand, consideration of the full pattern of loadings shows that the Alienation symptoms have a very different pattern of correlations than do the other three symptoms that correlate highly with Factor 4; namely Dysphoric Mood, Irritability, and Fatigue. These three symptoms are classic symptoms for depression. In particular, these three symptoms all correlate much more highly with Factor 3 (the other Depression factor) than do the Alienation symptoms. All five symptoms that correlate highly with Factor 3 are classic symptoms of depression. However, while Anhedonia and feelings of Hopelessness are specific to depression, the other three symptoms are also fi'equently found in other disorders. Furthermore, while the firll pattern of loadings is extremely similar for state Guilt, Appetite Disturbance, and Suicidality; the other two symptoms are not similar. This is especially clear in regard to the other depression factor, Factor 4. Anhedonia and feelings of Hopelessness correlate much more highly with Factor 4 than do Guilt, Appetite Disturbance, or Suicidality. Close examination of the symptoms highly correlated with Factors 3 and 4 suggests three clusters that might define syndromes. First, the two symptoms that tap feelings of Alienation form a closely knit cluster. Second, the three symptoms of Guilt, Appetite Disturbance, and Suicidality seem to form a closely knit 82 cluster. Third, the other symptoms more closely tied to the definition of depression can be put into a cluster: namely Dysphoric Mood, Anhedonia, Fatigue, Irritability, and Hopelessness. That is, there is one cluster specifically suggested by Factor 4, one specifically suggested by Factor 3, and one cluster of items that are very highly correlated with both factors. Initial Confirmatory Fact—or Analysfi The provisional clusters suggested by the exploratory factor analysis were tested by confirmatory factor analysis. The clusters are shown in Table 11. The confirmatory factor analysis showed that most of these symptom clusters are unidimensional and can be used to define a syndrome. However there were clear departures fi'om simple equivalence for both the Anxiety and Depression clusters. Anxiety subclusters. Close examination of the Anxiety cluster showed that the four symptoms can be subdivided into two pairs. The two arousal symptoms have much lower correlations with Depression than do the two cognitive symptoms. Thus for further analysis, the Anxiety cluster was split into two Anxiety subclusters. Autonomic Hyperarousal and Motor Tension were put in one cluster while, Fear and Vigilance were put in the other. Since the arousal symptoms have relatively low correlations with Depression, that cluster is called "Pure Anxiety" while the other cluster is called "Mixed Anxiety." Mon subclusters. Close examination of the Depression cluster showed that the five symptoms can be subdivided into two subclusters. Fatigue, Irritability, and 83 Table 11 Provisional Clusters Suggested by Exploratory Factor Analysis CLUSTER SYMPTOM Anxiety Autonomic Hyperarousal Motor Tension Fear (apprehension) Vigilance (psychological tension) Depression Dysphoric Mood Anhedonia Fatigue Irritability Hopelessness Severe Symptoms State Guilt Appetite Disturbance Suicidality Alienation Alienated Self-Conscious Need for Others Need for Approval Dependency Shame Mastery (reverse scored) Rosenberg Self-Esteem (reverse scored) Self-Esteem (reverse scored) Shame Self-Criticism 84 Table 1 1 (cont’d). CLUSTER SYMPTOM Insecurity Insufficiency Unstable Insecurity Hostility Hostility Hopelessness have relatively lower correlations with Anxiety than do Dysphoric Mood and Anhedonia. Thus for fiirther analysis, the Depression cluster was split into two subclusters. Fatigue, Irritability, and Hopelessness were put in one subcluster, while Dysphoric Mood and Anhedonia were put in the other. Since the energy symptoms have relatively low correlations with Anxiety, that cluster is called "Pure Depression" while the more directly mood related cluster is called "Mixed Depression". The 10 Working Clusters. The subdivision of the large Anxiety and Depression clusters produced a set of 10 provisional clusters to be considered as syndromes. This set of clusters was tested using confirmatory factor analysis. The factor loadings for the confirmatory factor analysis using the symptom construct correlations are reported in Table 12. This analysis was extended to include the three symptoms that were not used in the definition of the confirmatory factor clusters. The results for the unused symptoms are shown in Table 13. Examination of the results shows good fit for the unidimensionality of each cluster. 85 Table 12 The Confirmatory Factor Loadings for Construct Correlation Matrix Defined by 10 Symptom Clusters.* Factor/ CLUSTERS CONFIRMATORY FACTORS Pure Mixed Mixed Pure Shame Need for Severe insecurity Hostility Alienated Anxiety Ninety w W Others 8y“ Pure Anxrety MOTOR 98 95 85 79 57 29 74 47 79 81 TENSION AUTONOMIC 98 82 77 71 47 15 73 34 78 69 HYPERAROUSAL Mixed Anxiety FEAR 85 90 86 81 65 37 76 54 76 81 VIGH-ANCE 77 9O 79 76 48 47 49 54 63 82 Mixed Depression DYSPHORIC 73 87 87 92 62 36 69 59 65 88 MOOD WWW 7O 73 87 88 67 12 84 49 74 73 Pure Depression FANGUE 74 80 94 94 63 35 65 55 68 8O WARM“ 75 84 99 101 71 42 71 65 85 92 HOPELF—SSNESS 65 81 99 85 78 35 76 63 63 72 Shame SELF-ESTEEM 43 so 59 62 99 28 54 63 48 55 (ROSENBERG) MASTERY 35 47 55 58 9O 18 54 55 42 42 SELF-ESTES“ 44 58 65 67 85 41 49 69 52 65 SHAME 59 S7 73 68 89 5 68 45 62 67 SELF-CRITICAL 59 68 84 86 86 34 75 69 65 73 ' All decimals omitted. 86 Table 12 (cont’d). Factor/ CLUSTERS CONFIRMATORY FACTORS Pure Mixed Mixed Pure Shame Nwd for Severe Insecurity Hostility Alienated Anxiety Anxiety w burn-i- Others 9mm- Need for Others NEED/ APPROVAL DEPENDENC Y Severe Symptoms GUILT SUICIDALITY APPETITE. Insecurity UNSTABLE INSUFFICIENCY INSECURE Hostility HOSTILITY Alienation ALIENATED SELF—CONSCIOUS ‘ All decimals omitted. 87 Table 13 The Confirmatory Factor Loadings for the Symptoms Not Used to Define Clusters in the Analysis of the Symptom Construct Correlation Matrix Defined by 10 Symptom Clusters.* CONFIRMATORY RESIDUAL (i.e., unused) ITEMS FACTORS Sleep Efficacy Guilt Pure Anxiety 44 ll 30 Mixed Anxiety 40 ~5 37 Mixed Depression 47 13 37 Pure Depression 46 12 39 Shame 36 42 44 Need for Others 10 ~53 47 Severe Symptoms 41 29 31 Insecurity 25 ~12 53 Hostility 42 15 33 Alienation 42 1 41 ‘ All decimals omitted. 88 That is, the symptoms within each cluster follow the pattern required for statistical equivalence. Since the clusters delineate unidimensional sets of symptoms, the confirmatory factor model shows good fit. Thus, essentially all of the information in the symptom profile is captured by the 10 confirmatory constructs. These will be provisionally called "syndromes" for short. The correlations between syndromes are shown in Table 14. One correlation shows a problem in differential validity: the estimated correlation of 1.04 between the two Depression subclusters. The fact that this correlation is larger than 1.00 is due to sampling error in the estimation process. However, if we revise that estimated correlation down to 1.00 it still poses a problem. Is there any difference between the two Depression subclusters? There are two ways in which the subclusters differ. First, they differ in their correlations with Anxiety. For Pure Anxiety, the correlations are .77 for the Pure Depression factor and .83 for the Mixed Depression factor (See Table 15). For Mixed Anxiety, the correlations are .87 for the Pure Depression factor and .92 for the Mixed Depression factor. Second, the two factors differ in the extent of correlation with Need for Others, where the correlations are .40 for the Pure Depression factor and .28 for the Mixed Depression factor. So there is some evidence for differentiation between the two depression subclusters. If they are different, then it would mean that the estimated correlation of 1.04 is a sampling error departure from some number smaller than 1.00. This is certainly 89 Table 14 Correlations Between Syndromes“ SYNDROME 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 1. Pure Anxiety 100 91 83 77 53 23 75 42 80 77 2. Mixed Anxiety 91 100 92 87 63 47 69 60 78 91 3. Mixed Depression 83 92 100 104 75 28 89 62 80 93 4. Pure Depression 77 87 104 100 76 40 76 65 77 87 5. Shame 53 63 75 76 100 29 67 67 6O 67 6. Need for Others 23 47 28 40 29 100 7 67 17 54 7. Severe Symptoms 75 69 89 76 67 7 100 30 71 60 8. Insecurity 42 6O 62 65 67 67 30 100 42 62 9. Hostility 80 78 8O 77 60 17 71 42 100 73 1‘0. Alienation 77 91 93 87 67 54 60 62 73 100 " All decimals omitted. possible with the sample size for this study though there is no way to estimate just how high the population correlation might be. If we assume a two dimensional structure for the four subclusters, it is possible to generate an algebraic estimate of the correlation between the Depression clusters from the other five correlations. That estimate is 1_' =94. This value is within the confidence interval around 1.04 and is thus consistent with the data. There are some other very high correlations in this table as might be predicted fiom the exploratory factor findings. There is a very high correlation of .92 between Alienation and Mixed Depression. There is a very high correlation of .89 between the 90 Severe Symptoms factor and Mixed Depression. Is there a difference between Mixed Depression, Alienation, and the Severe Symptoms factors? One sharp difference is in how the three factors relate to Need for Others (See Table 15). Table 15 Correlations Between Need for Others and Mixed Depression, Severe Symptoms, and Alienation SYMPTOM Need for Others Mixed Depression .28 Severe Symptoms .07 Alienation .54 The difference between r = .54 for Alienation and r = .07 for the Severe Symptoms factor is very large. Furthermore, while both Alienation and the Severe Symptoms factor have very high correlations with Mixed Depression, they have a much lower level of correlation with each other: ; = .60. There is another difference between the Severe Symptoms factor and Mixed Depression. The Mixed Depression factor shows a large difference in its correlations with Pure Depression (nominal ; = 1.04; estimated [ = .94) and with Pure Anxiety (I = .83). The Severe Symptoms factor shows little difference in its correlations with Pure Depression (I = .76) and with Pure Anxiety (1 = .75). 91 The Smptom Correlations Of the 28 symptoms that could be measured in this study, 25 were used to define syndromal clusters and 3 were not used. Thus, the symptom correlation matrix is a bulky 25 x 25 in the primary symptoms are considered and a bulky 28 x 28 if all symptoms are considered. The symptom correlations are much easier to look at if the symptoms are organized. The good fit for the confirmatory factor model shows that the 10 working clusters provide a good way to organize the clusters. Symptoms can be correlated at one of two levels. The items in each cluster can be used to generate a scale to measure that symptom. These scale correlations are attenuated by error of measurement in each cluster. The key to reducing error is to use many items. This was not possible in this study since so many different symptoms were considered. For small clusters, the scale will have low reliability and the scale correlations for that scale will be very much attenuated. Confirmatory factor analysis can be used to estimate the size of the correlation between symptom constructs themselves. These are the correlations that are assumed mentally when we think about "the correlation" between two symptoms in terms of theory or clinical practice. That is, confirmatory factor analysis estimates the size of the correlation that would be obtained from a perfect measure of the symptom: i.e., the correlation if we could measure each symptom with a very long scale. These construct correlations are the same as the scale correlations corrected for attenuation using the scale reliabilities. 92 Table 16 presents the symptom correlation matrix organized by clusters. Table 18 presents the symptom scale correlation matrix; i.e., the size of the correlations for the imperfect symptom measures in this study. Scatter plots of the total scores for anxiety and depression for each individual revealed an extreme skew which can not be discerned from the correlational data. This skew was more extreme for the anxiety scores than for the depression scores. It is possible that we are looking at one end of a normal distribution. If so, a more balanced scale, in which both ends of the continuum were represented, would show a clearer picture of the actual constructs. Unfortunately, in the current study, those items which might best describe the positive end of a continuum moving from fatigue to energy are best represented by items from the GTS, which is wrought with measurement problems. It is not clear at this point whether anxiety can best be described as a continuum with contentment at the opposite pole, or whether anxiety may be better conceptualized as a unipolar construct in which contentment, or relaxation, is merely the absence of anxiety. Anxiety Driven Depression The van Praag (1994) theory of serotonin driven depression predicts that high trait anxiety will produce high trait depression. However, because of the effect of serotonin dysregulation on hostility and aggression, his theory also predicts an equally strong relationship between hostility and depression. This prediction can be tested using multiple regression. The multiple regression analysis for the present data is presented in Table 18. 93 8 2 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 Ma 8 8 2. 2. z 8 8 2. 2. 2. 8 2. 8 8 326.3 .8 z 2. 8 8 8 8 8 8 a 8 8 8 a :. 8 8 2. 2. 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8:8 .8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 2. 8 8 8 8 8 8 2. z. 2. 2. 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 38.8.28 .8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 .8 .8 8 8 8 8 8 2. 2. 2. 8 8 8 8 8 .8 8 8 8 8 8 tan: .8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 9.3863882.» .8 8.38 2 2 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 2. 2. 2. 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 2. 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 all}: .8 8 . 3 2. a 8 8 2. 8 8 8 z. 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8. 8 2. 8 2. 2. 8 8 £335 .8 z o 8 8 2. 8 8 8 8 8 8 2. 8 8 2. 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 2. 2. 2. z. .88.. .: zoanuuaouui 8 2 8 8 8 2. 8 8 8 a 2. 8 8 o 8 8 8 8 8 8 2. 8 2. 2. 8 2. 8 2. g .8 8 . 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 2. 2. 2. 2. 8 2. 8.: 38.1.8 .2 zoaganaaa 8 z. 8 2. 2. 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 2. 2. 8 . 2. 8 8 8 8 88.25 .8 8 a 8 8 8 2. 8 8 8 8 2. 2. 8 8 2. 8 8 8 8 8 2. 2. . 2. 2. 8 8 2. 8 a... ._ Egan: 8 2 8 8 8 2. 8 8 a 8 8 8 2 2 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 2. 8 8 8 2. 8 8 1358:3583 .n 8 a 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 2. 8 2. 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 z. 2. 2. 8 8 8 8 82.3.88: 8 Egg 8 8 z 8 8 8 8 2 8 2 8 8 2.. 2 8 8 8 8 8 8 n : 8 8 v _ n « 528255.30 2282 228—280 88885 aneim $282233. .8 2202.53 2 035. 94 . 8. : 8 8 8 8 8n .8 8 8 8 .8 2.8 8 8 8 .8 8 8 88 8 8 8 8 8 8 8... 2.85353 .88 8. . 8. 2 a 2 2. 2.. 8.. 8 8 2 2. 8. 2 2. 8 8 8n 2 8 a 2 8 2. n 2 8 8852.8 .8 .. 8. a 8 8 8 8 8 .w 8 8 8 8. 8 .8 8 8 8 8 8 .8 8 8 .8 8 8 8 88 asp-32......» .8.. 53982. 8 8 8 8. 82 8 8 8 .8 8 2.8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 2. 8 8 8 2. 8 8 2. nan-8.2.8 8 2 2. 8 8. 8. 8 8 8 8 82 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 2.8 8 8 2. 8 8 2. 8 8 2. 3.8.2 .8 2058882.? 8 2 8 8 8 8. 8 ...8 8 2. 88 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 82. 8 .8 2. 2. 2. 2.22.18. .8 22.588: 8 2. 8 8 8 8 8. 8 8 8 8 8 . 2. 28 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 58a. .8 8 2.. 8 8 8 8 8 ... 8 8 8 88 82. 8 8 8. 82. 8 8 8 .2. 8 8... 8 8 8 2.8 8 22.-18...... .2 8 ... 2 8 8 8 8 8 8. 8. 2.. 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 835...: .88 22.22582... 8 8 8 8 82 2. .2 8 8. 2: a 8. 2 n. 8 8 8 8 8 2.2. 2. 8 8 8 8 8 8 2. 8.1.323 .2 8 8 8 8 8 8.. 8 8 2.. .2 2. 8 8 8. 8 8 2.8 8 8 2. 8 8 22. 8 2.8 82. 8 8 2.33.8 .8 8 2 8 8 8 8 8 88 8 8. 8 8 2.. 2. 8. 8 8 8 8m 2. 82. 2. 8. 8 8 2. 8 .2. 2.6.18 .8 88023822826» .8 2. 8. 8 8 8 2. 82. 8 2 8 2.. 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 88 8 8 8 8 2 8 8382.2 .2.. 8 8.8 8 8 8 2 8 8 8 8. 8. 2. 8 8 8 ... 8 8 8 8 8 8 2. 8 8 8 2 8 388.8823: .2 8.25.29.23.22 88 8 8. 8 8 8 8 2 8 2 .n 8 2.. 2 8 8 8 8 .8 8 n 2 8 2 8 . n 8 332.82.23.33 6.28. o. 28... 95 3 2 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 2 3 3 3 3 3. 3 3 3 .3 3 3 3 3 3 3304...“. .8 2 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 2 3 3 3 2 o. 3 .3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 and .3 3 2 3 3 3. 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 .3 3 3 3 3 3855.. .3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 2 3 3 3 3 3 2. 3 3 3 3 3 R 3 3 3 ban: .3 3 z z 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 2 3 3 2. 2. 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 938863835.» .3 3.33 2 a 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 .: 3 3 3 3 3 3. 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3.9.2.: .3. 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 2 3 3 3 3 .3 3 3. 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 33.33 .3 3 3 3 .3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 2 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 .3 3 ...-u... .: 293.533: 2 2 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 2 o 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 . 3 3 383.2 .3 z 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 2.. 2 3 3 3. .3 3 3 3 3 .3 3 3 3 3 3 3.23338 2 20335938: 3 .. 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 .3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 8.335 .3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 8 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 .... ._ Egan: : 3 3 .3 .3 s 2 3 2 3 3 3. z o 3 3 3 3 3. 3 .3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 1:253:32 .n 3 o 3 3 3 2. 3 3 .n 3 3 3 3 z 3 3 3 on 3 3 3. 3 3 O S F 3 8 833.584 .u tugs—2 3 3 z 3 3 3 3 2 3 2 3 3 2 2 3 3 .3 3 3 .3 2 3 2 3 2 3 . .n u it; €232.82 Sm 362.50 875 5.53.4 noun—0300 2.8m Boa—gm E 035. 96 on _ n. 2 3 3 3 on R 3 2 3 3 3 3 2 2 3 on 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 2.33.383 .3 a. . : _ o : 3. ... a. 2 2 2 8 2. 2 3 2 3 3 g a. 3 2 n a. u a . b.8553 .3 2 : 2. 3 3 3 2 3 z 3 3 3 : n 3 3 3 3 3 .3 3 3 3. 3 an 3 3 3 8.333993» .3. .5393 a _ 3 8 3 2 2 av. 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 «a 3 3 3 3 3 3. 3 3 3 a. 3 31.883 .3 a o 2 8 3 2 .n 3 .n 3 3 3 3 3 a 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 a c. 3 3 3 3 3:32 .3 29.5533 3 : 3 2 2 8. 2 on 3 3 3 3 2 n a 3 3 3 3 3 3 a 3 2 on 3 8 3 $39: .3 E33: 3 3. 2 on .n a .2 3 3 2 3 3. 3 3 3 «n 3 3. 3 3 a 2.. 3 3 3 3 R 3 a 23.... .3 a 2.. 3 3 3 2 3 2 a 2 3 3 3 3 3 2.. 3 3 3 3 an 3.. 3 3 .7 3 3 3 33 .2 2 a. z 3 z 2 e a 8 n. : z a 3 a 2 3 3 3 3 an 3 3 3 a 3 .2 3 .335 3 E5082. 2 2 3 2 3 3 2 2 n. 3 3 3 o u. 3 2 .3 3 3 3 8 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 again; .2 2 2 3 n 3 3 8 3 : 3 a a 3 _. z 3 3 3 3 3 3 8 .9. 8 3 8 3 3 5333 .3 3 2 3 a 3 3 3 3 z 3 a 3 2 n a S 3 3 3 3 3 3 a 3 on 3 3 3 5.6.16 .3 ”20.53533” .2 2. : a 2 : c 3 z o 3 s 3 e 3 z 3 2 3 3 3 8 2 3 a on 2 3 38.3899 .2 a 2. n 3 3 n 3 3 3 n. _. n 3 3 2 o. 3 3 2 2 2 2 o 2 3 3 3 2 13.52.2382 .2 gueauz 3 3 z 2 3 3 3 2 3 2 3 3 2 2 3 3 3 3 3. 3 n : 3 2 v . ... 3 Egg €253 : 2.3 97 Table 18 Correlations Between Pure Anxiety, Hostility, and Pure Depression'“ Pure Anxiety Hostility Pure Depression Pure Anxiety 100 Hostility 80 100 Pure Depression 77 77 100 ‘ All decimals omitted. Multiple regression: Beta for anxiety: .43 Beta for hostility: .43 Multiple correlation: .81 The multiple correlation is consistent with the prediction derived fi'om the van Praag (1994) theory. The correlation between Hostility and Pure Depression is as high as the correlation between Pure Anxiety and Depression. The beta weight for Hostility is just as high as that for Pure Anxiety. The multiple correlation increases from a high .77 to a still higher .81. Results Pertaining to a One-Factor Model One Higher Order Factor? Some advocates of the Big Five factor model have argued that symptoms considered in this study are largely expressions of a single factor called “neuroticism” or “negative affectivity.” This hypothesis can be tested statistically at two levels. Those tests 98 will be considered in this segment. The results will show that considerable information would be lost if the symptoms were replaced by one global factor. The strongest test of the one-factor model would consider the prediction of variables outside the domain of neuroticism. No such variables were measured for this study, and so the strong test of this model cannot be performed here. However, weaker tests can be done which consider just the structure of correlations between items, between symptoms, or between syndromes. It will be shown that the one factor model fails using even these weaker tests. The FlaAOne-Factor Model Many researchers have argued that the symptoms collected for this study are largely expressions of one underlying factor, traditionally called “neuroticism.” This model can be tested statistically. The flat model makes no distinction between symptoms. Thus, the unit of analysis for this model is the item. That is, the flat model predicts that each item differs only by random error from the single dimension of neuroticism. If this model were true, then all of the symptom clusters would be equivalent to one another. For any two symptoms, the population construct correlation would be 1.00. The average sample construct correlation would thus be 1.00. Half of the sample correlations would be expected to be larger than 1.00, and half would be expected to be smaller. The symptoms construct correlation matrix was presented in Table 14. There is one sample correlation between Mixed Depression and Pure Depression which is 1.04. 99 All other correlations are less than 1. The correlation between Need for Others and the Severe Symptom cluster is only .07. The average correlation between symptoms is only .65; far less than 1.00. The flat factor model is clearly disconfinned. The Hierarchical One-Factor Model Big Five theorists have a more sophisticated model which is hierarchical in structure. This model acknowledges that items in the domain of neuroticism form clusters such that the correlations within each cluster are larger than would be predicted by the general factor of neuroticism. The factor defined by each cluster is called a “facet.” The theorists acknowledge that facets have specific variation above and beyond variation due to the general factor. However, they argue that the only significant variation in the facets is the variation due to the general factor. In the data for this study, there are actually two lower levels considered, not just one. First, items can be collected in clusters that define symptoms. Second, the symptoms can be clustered to form syndromes. There is no recognition of this double level of structure in the current Big Five literature. The hierarchical factor model can be tested at either level. That is, the concept of “facet” can be considered where “facet” denotes “symptom” or where facet denotes “syndrome.” The model was tested at both levels and the results follow. Test at the symptom level. Suppose that the concept “facet” is considered to mean “symptom.” In this study I identified 28 measurable symptoms. If the word “facet” is 100 defined by symptoms, then the hierarchical one factor model would claim that the symptom correlation matrix should be explained by one factor. That is, if all significant variation in facets is explained by neuroticism, then the size of the correlations between the facets must be explained by the general factor. This hypothesis can be tested using the data from the current study. Consider first the implications for the factor analysis of the symptom correlation matrix. According to the one factor model, the symptom correlation matrix should have only one common factor. The exploratory factor analysis reported in Table 10 found 10 factors, where 6 factors had high correlations with more than one symptom. That disconfirrns the one factor model. It is mathematically possible for the multiple factor structure in the data to be due to sampling error. In clinical psychology and psychiatry, there are some studies with samples so small that this becomes a real concern. In this study, the sample size was I)! = 366, which makes this concern unreasonable. However, this remote possibility can be tested statistically by using confirmatory factor analysis to do a significance test on the departures from the predictions of the one, factor model. This was done using the symptom scale correlation matrix. The deviations from the one factor model generated a chi square value of 33340.53 with 377 degrees of freedom. This is equivalent to a _2_ value of 108, and is significant at far beyond the .000001 level. Thus, the one factor model is totally disconfirmed at the symptom level. The deviations from the one factor model are statistically massive and totally lOl beyond question. Are the deviations substantively significant? In this segment, the substantive meaning of the deviations is not considered. However, it is possible to consider simply the size of the deviations. Are the deviations large enough to be of practical significance? An abstract way to approach this question is through partial correlation. The one factor model is fitted to the data and a correlation is computed between each symptom and the general factor. The factor correlations can then be used to compute partial correlations. If the one factor model were true, then each such partial correlation would be 0 if computed for population data. The sample partial correlation - would differ from 0 only by sampling error. The partial correlations were computed for the symptom scale correlation matrix. Ifthe population partial correlation is 0, and the sample size is E = 330, the standard error of the sample population correlations is .055. The probability of a sample correlation as large as .20 is only .00014, or 1 in 7143. Yet, among the 378 symptom partial correlations, there were 76 correlations larger than .20, of which 24 were larger than .30, 10 were larger than .40, and 5 were larger than .50. Lipsey and Wilson (1993) compiled meta-analyses across 18,000 treatment studies in psychology, and found an average treatment correlation of r = .23. By that standard, 76 of the 378 partial correlations were large enough to be of practical significance. Thus, considerable information would have been lost if the symptoms were replaced by a single neuroticism factor. 102 Test at the syndrome level. Examination of the content names for the facets used in the current Big Five inventories shows names like anxiety, depression, self- consciousness, etc. Thus, the facets currently used are closer in level to syndromes than to symptoms. The one factor model can also be tested at the syndrome level. If syndromes are the “facets” of the hierarchical model, then the syndrome correlation matrix should be explained by one factor. That is, if all significant variation in facets is explained by neuroticism, then the size of the correlations between the facets must be explained by the general factor. This hypothesis can be tested using the data from this study. Consider first the implications for the factor analysis of the syndrome correlation matrix. According to the one factor model, the syndrome correlation matrix should have only one common factor. To test this hypothesis, the syndrome scale matrix was computed, and confirmatory factor analysis was used to fit a one factor model to the data. The deviations from the one factor model generated a chi square value of 357.35 with 44 degrees of freedom. This is equivalent to a ; value of 33.40, which is significant at far beyond the .0001 level. Thus, the one factor model is disconfirrned at the syndrome level. The deviations from the one factor model are statistically massive and beyond question. Are the deviations large enough to be of practical significance? An abstract way to approach this question is to compute the partial correlations between syndromes with the general factor held constant. If the one factor model were true, then each such partial correlation would be 0 if computed for population data. The sample partial 103 correlation would differ from 0 only by sampling error. The partial correlations were computed for the syndrome scale correlation matrix. If the population partial correlation is 0, and the sample size is E = 330, the standard error of the sample partial correlations is .055. The probability of a sample correlation as large as .20 is only .00014, or 1 in 7143. Yet, among the 45 syndrome partial correlations, there were 12 correlations larger than .20, two of which were larger than .30. Using the average treatment correlation of .23 found by Lipsey and Wilson (1993), 12 of the 45 partial correlations were large enough to be of practical significance. Thus, considerable information would be lost if the syndromes were replaced by a single neuroticism factor. The one factor model does not fit the data. The flat item model fails; the hierarchical model using symptoms as facets fails; and the hierarchical model using syndromes as facets fails. Thus, no substantive construct was found that corresponds to the concept of “neuroticism.” On the other hand, the correlations between syndromes are all positive: some quite large. So it is possible to define mathematical general factors which are composites of the neurotic syndromes. Since the one factor model does not fit the data, the mathematical factor is not uniquely defined and the exact content of the factor will vary somewhat from one study to another, depending on the specific set of syndromes considered in any given study. However, the neuroticism factors defined in different studies will be very highly correlated with one another. 104 This mathematical neuroticism factor provides a useful first approximation to the data and may be all that is needed in some applications. A person who is high on the mathematical neuroticism factor will tend to be high on all of the symptoms; however, that individual will be higher on those symptoms more highly correlated with the neuroticism factor than on those symptoms less highly correlated with the neuroticism factor. Since the one factor model does not fit the data, there may be no substantive neuroticism factor as such. On the other hand, there may be a substantive neuroticism factor which is only one cause of the high correlations between certain symptoms. However, there are now many research findings that show that many of the high correlations between symptoms and syndromes are due to causal rather than structural relations. In view of that fact, researchers should exercise extreme caution in using “neuroticism” as an explanatory variable. Use of such terminology must be regarded as speculative at this point. CHAPTER 6 DISCUSSION In this study, I looked at clusters of symptoms associated with anxiety and depression in an attempt to better understand commonalities and distinctions between these disorders. The literature suggests that depression should be linked with dysphoric mood, low self-esteem, indecisiveness, hopelessness, psychomotor retardation, anhedonia, feelings of guilt or worthlessness, and suicidal ideation. Those symptoms most specifically linked to anxiety have been feelings of worry, anxiety, disquietude, and irritability; fears of dying or being out of control; somatic symptoms, such as shortness of breath, dizziness, chest pains, aches, and an exaggerated startle response; depersonalization and derealization; and shyness. Those symptoms linked to both depression and anxiety include appetite disturbance and abdominal distress, sleep disturbance, fatiguability, concentration difficulties, and agitation. The current findings support some of these expected links, but not others. Depression was linked most specifically to fatigue and hopelessness, as anticipated, but also linked to dysphoric mood and anhedonia. Anxiety was linked most specifically to somatic symptoms of motor tension and autonomic hyperarousal, and also to fear and vigilance. This replicates previous findings linking somatization to anxiety, but not to depression (King, Margraf, Ehlers, & Maddock, 1986). Contrary to expectations, 105 106 irritability and worry were linked to depression rather than anxiety. These findings will be discussed in greater detail in ensuing sections, with one exception; because of the problems associated with the Worry cluster, the findings regarding worry should be considered as tentative and will not be elaborated. It is notable that each syndrome was best distinguished by two clusters: one psychological and one physiological. Anxiety was best distinguished first by physiological symptoms associated with activation of the arousal system, and second by psychological symptoms associated with fear and vigilance. Depression was best distinguished by physiological symptoms associated with decreased energy, and by psychological symptoms associated with dysphoric mood and irritability. Mtive Affectivity: A General Distress Factor? At this point, we can begin to answer empirically questions about some of the models which have been presented. The findings from this study were in many ways highly consistent with those found by Watson and colleagues (1995b) ‘in their factor analysis of symptoms of anxiety and depression. However, it should be noted that neither my results nor their results were consistent with their conclusions. In the Watson and colleagues (1995b) study, the authors interpreted the high correlations between symptoms of anxiety and depression as evidence of a general distress factor. However, their results showed no evidence for a separate general distress factor. Rather, the symptoms that they placed in the General Distress category each follow one of two patterns: the pattern for the anxiety items or the pattern for the depression items. In terms of those items which 107 were most discriminative, Watson and colleagues (1995a) found that those items best distinguishing anxiety fiom depression were symptomatic of “Anxious Arousal.” Items which Watson and colleagues (1995a) claimed best distinguished depression from anxiety were best characterized by “Loss of Interest” and “High Positive Affect.” Their interpretation of depression as anhedonia led them to incorporate the High Positive Affect items into their design. However, their findings show that the High Positive Affect items are no more highly correlated with loss of interest than with the other depression items. A perusal of the factor loadings of the items used by Watson and colleagues (1995b) showed that their “General Distress” items were better markers of either anxiety or depression than they were for a general distress factor. A content analysis of Watson and colleagues’ (1995b) items revealed a great deal of overlap across the categories that they formed (See Table 19). This is not surprising as the categories were formed on the basis of lists of items thought to be linked to the disorder in clinical practice, rather than on the basis of either a content analysis or their empirical loadings. The “Anxious Arousal” factor appears to be the cleanest, consisting of 12 items associated with autonomic hyperarousal, four items associated with motor tension, and one item associated with fear. The “Loss of Interest” category had four Anhedonia items, two items measuring Fatigue, one Suicidality item, and one Shame item (measuring Self- Criticism). The “General Distress: Anxious Symptoms” category had four items measuring 108 Table 1.9 Content analysis of the Items Considered in the Watson and Colleagues (1995b) Study. SCALE / Item from Watson et al., 1995b Symptom Category from Current Study GENERAL DISTRESS: MIXED SYMPTOMS Worried a lot about things Worry Trouble concentrating Cognitive Disturbance Felt dissatisfied with things Self-Critical Felt confused Cognitive Disturbance Felt irritable Irritability Trouble making decisions Cognitive Disturbance Trouble paying attention Cognitive Disturbance Felt restless Motor Tension Felt something awful would happen Fear Got fatigued easily Fatigue Trouble remembering things Cognitive Disturbance Trouble falling asleep Sleep Disturbance Trouble staying asleep Sleep Disturbance Loss of appetite Appetite Disturbance Slept very well (-) Sleep Disturbance GENERAL DISTRESS: DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS Felt depressed Dysphoric Mood Felt discouraged Self-C ritical Felt sad Dysphoric Mood Felt hopeless Hopelessness Disappointed in myself Self-Critical Felt like crying Dysphoric Mood Felt like a failure Self-Critical Felt worthless Self-Esteem (-) 109 Table 19 (cont’d). SCALE I Item from Watson et al., 1995b Symptom Category from Current Study GENERAL DISTRESS: DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS (Cont’d) Blamed myself for things Guilt Felt inferior to others Self-Esteem Pessimistic about the future Hopelessness Felt tired or sluggish Fatigue GENERAL DISTRESS: ANXIOUS SYMPTOMS Felt tense: “high strung” Vigilance Felt uneasy too vague to categorize Felt nervous Vigilance Felt afraid Fear Felt “on edge,” keyed up Vigilance Unable to relax Vigilance Lump in my throat Autonomic Hyperarousal Upset stomach Autonomic Hyperarousal Tense or sore muscles Motor Tension Felt nauseous Autonomic Hyperarousal Had diarrhea Autonomic Hyperarousal LOSS OF INTEREST Felt unattractive Self-Critical Felt that nothing was enjoyable Anhedonia Felt withdrawn from others Anhedonia Took extra efl'ort to get started Fatigue Felt slowed down Fatigue Nothing was interesting or fun Anhedonia Felt bored Anhedonia Thought about death, suicide Suicidality 110 Table 19 (cont’d). SCALE / Item from Watson et al., 1995b Symptom Category from Current Study ANXIOUS AROUSAL Felt dizzy, lightheaded Autonomic Hyperarousal Was trembling, shaking Motor Tension Shaky hands Motor Tension Trouble swallowing Autonomic Hyperarousal Short of breath Autonomic Hyperarousal Dry mouth Autonomic Hyperarousal Twitching or trembling muscles Motor Tension Hot or cold spells Autonomic Hyperarousal Cold or sweaty hands Autonomic Hyperarousal Felt like I was choking Autonomic Hyperarousal Felt faint Autonomic Hyperarousal Pain in chest Motor Tension Racing or pounding heart Autonomic Hyperarousal Felt numbness or tingling Autonomic Hyperarousal Afraid I was going to die Fear Had to urinate frequently Autonomic Hyperarousal Easily startled Autonomic Hyperarousal HIGH POSITIVE AFFECT Felt really lively, “up” ’ Dysphoric Mood (-) Felt really happy ‘ Dysphoric Mood (-) Felt I had a lot of energy ‘ Fatigue (-) Was having a lot of fun ‘ Anhedonia (-) Felt I had much to look forward to ’ Hopelessness (-) Felt good about myself ‘ Self-Esteem I had many interesting things to do ‘ Anhedonia (-) 111 Table 19 (cont’d). SCALE / Item from Watson et al., 1995b Symptom Category from Current Study HIGH POSITIVE AFFECT (Cont’d) Felt confident Self-Esteem Looked forward to things ‘ Hopelessness (-) Felt I had accomplished a lot ‘ Self-Critical (-) Was proud of myself ‘ Self-Critical (-) Felt cheerful ' Dysphoric Mood (-) Felt successful Self-Critical (') Felt optimistic ' Howlessncss (-) Felt really talkative Anhedonia (-) Moved quickly and easily ‘ Fatigue (-) Felt hopeful about the future ' Hopelessness (-) Able to laugh easily Dysphoric Mood (-) Felt like being with others Anhedonia (-) Felt very clearheaded Cognitive Disturbance (-) Thoughts came to me very easily Cognitive Disturbance (-) Felt very alert Fatiguc (-) Could do everything I needed to Anhedonia (-) Felt I didn’t need much sleep Fatigue (-) (-) Reverse scored. ' Selected as reverse keyed item for the Anhedonic Depression scale by Watson et al, 1995b. 112 Autonomic Hyperarousal, one item measuring Motor Tension, four items measuring Vigilance, one item measuring Fear, and one final item (“felt uneasy”) that was too vague to categorize effectively. The “General Distress: Depressive Symptoms” category had five items measuring Shame (three measuring Self-Criticism and two measuring low Self-Esteem), three items measuring Dysphoric Mood, two measuring Hopelessness, and one measuring Guilt. The “General Distress” category had 15 items: five measuring Cognitive Disturbance, two measuring Depression (one Fatigue item and one Irritability item), one measuring Shame, one measuring Worry, one measuring Appetite Disturbance, and one measuring Sleep Disturbance. They also had a category that is radically different in content from the symptoms usually listed for anxiety and depression: the “High Positive Affect” category (which they also termed “Anhedonic Depression”) had 14 items. These items were designed to measure feelings of happiness, energy, and positive self-regard. The items can also be classified as the bipolar opposites of some of the symptoms for depression. Looking at the scale this way produced the following analysis: two items measuring Anhedonia, three measuring Fatigue, three measuring Hopelessness, three measuring Dysphoric Mood, and two measuring Shame (i.e., low Self-Esteem). Thus, of the 14 “Anhedonic Depression” items, only two items specifically consider Anhedonia; the other 12 tap different dimensions. Watson and colleagues (1995a) ultimately reverse scored the “High Positive 113 Affect” items and combined them with the “Loss of Interest” items to form a measure intended to be specific to depression and separate from General Distress. However, careful examination of the factor loadings shows that there is no basis for this combination. The remaining depression items correlate just as highly with the Positive Affect factor as do the “Loss of Interest” items. Furthermore, there is only a modest correlation between the “High Positive Affect” item construct and the depression item construct (r = -.50). However, it is possible that that correlation may be lowered due to nonlinearity. The real correlation may in actuality be much higher. Because strong markers of both anxiety and depression were included in the General Distress category, it is not surprising that it correlated highly with both anxiety and depression factors. However, invoking a “General Distress” category does little to tease out the salient components of depression and anxiety, once arousal and anhedonia have been factored out. A content analysis of the items within the categories supports the contention that there are two major categories of symptoms within the category of anxiety: the first characterized by primarily physiological symptoms associated with Autonomic Hyperarousal and Motor Tension, the second characterized by primarily psychological symptoms associated with Fear and Vigilance. In my study, when I split the original Anxiety cluster into two separate clusters, depending on the level of correlation, I found that the physiological symptoms defined one factor, which I termed “Pure” anxiety symptoms, whereas the psychological symptoms defined a second factor, which I termed “Mixed” symptoms of anxiety. 114 The construct of Depression appears to be somewhat more complex. When I broke down the Depressive symptoms into those which were relatively “Pure” versus those which were more “Mixed” markers of depression, I found that my results, in this case, were largely at odds with those of Watson and colleagues (1995b) in that their analysis did not distinguish between anhedonia and fatigue. To the contrary, my best markers of “Pure” Depression were the clusters of Fatigue, Irritability, and Hopelessness, whereas the superordinate cluster of “Mixed” Depression contained Dysphoric Mood, Anhedonia, and Worry. Notably, in the current study the high correlation between the categories of “Pure” and “Mixed” Depression appears to be due largely to the high correlations between the various variants of dysphoric mood: Dysphoric Mood, Wony, and Irritability, rather than the relatively lower correlations between Fatigue and Anhedonia. Results fi'om the current study do not support Watson and Clark’s conceptualization of depression. Consistent with my own data, Watson and his colleagues (1995b) found that items associated with Shame, such as self-criticism and low self-esteem, tended to correlate more highly with items associated with depression than with items associated with anxiety. However, including these items within their “General Distress: Depressive Symptoms” may be a poor solution. My findings support a correlation between Shame and Depression (r = .76); however this correlation is low enough that the two constructs may best be conceptualized as distinct and separate. The results fi'om the current study suggest that there are meaningful distinctions 115 between subjective experiences of anxiety and depression in spite of their similarities. These differences may be obscured when “General Distress” is invoked as a means for explaining those similarities. Confirmaticmnd Disconfinnation of Clalrrs The “Interpersonal Sensitivity” Scale The “Interpersonal Sensitivity” scale of the SCL-90 purports to measure what has often been called “rejection sensitivity” in the literature. This construct is often depicted as an aspect of depression (Blatt, 1974), and therefore could be presumed to correlate more highly with depression than with anxiety. However, the “Interpersonal Sensitivity” scale is not homogeneous. A content analysis of its items shows that three concede feelings of being unliked or “Alienated” from others. Three of the items may best be described as “Self-Conscious.” One item described feeling critical of others, consistent with items describing “Hostility.” The remaining items did not clearly fit with any of the other symptoms, and were not included in further analyses. The “Interpersonal Sensitivity” scale was also not internally valid; the content did not fit the title. If this had been a valid measure of the construct, we would have expected higher correlations between “Interpersonal Sensitivity” and Depression than Anxiety. The actual differences found were modest: .85 versus .73. Depictions in the literature of rejection sensitivity as an important correlate of anaclitic depression (Blatt, Quinlan, & Chevron, 1990) would lead us to predict higher correlations between “Interpersonal Sensitivity” and Need for Others than Insecurity. This would be consistent with previous 116 findings that link the anaclitic style to feelings of being unliked or unpopular (Blatt, Hart, Quinlan, Leadbetter, & Auerbach, 1993). These expectations were not born out; “Interpersonal Sensitivity” was actually more strongly associated to Insecurity (the current study cluster composed of introjective items; 1 = .59) than to Need for Others (the cluster composed of anaclitic items; r = .47). We would also have expected to find higher correlations between the “Interpersonal Sensitivity” scale and Irritability and Hostility than were actually found (Becker & Lesiak, 1977). Taken together, these findings suggest that this scale is not a good measure of the construct. Irritability may be a better measure of rejection sensitivity than the symptom clusters contained within the “Interpersonal Sensitivity” scale of the SCL-90. Hostilig and Irritability High correlations between anxiety, depression, and correlates of anger, such as hostility and irritability, were found in the current study. These findings support previous work in which significant correlations of these three constructs were found at both the trait (Mook, van der Ploeg, & Kleijn (1990) and state levels (Gotlib & Meyer, 1986; Zuckerman, Persky, Eckman, & Hopkins, 1967). The high correlations found in the current study are more consistent with correlations reported at the state level, which tend to range between .67 and .87 (Gotlib & Meyer, 1986; Zuckerman, Persky, Eckrnan, & Hopkins, 1967), than with those found at the trait level, which tend to be somewhat lower (Mook, van der Ploeg, & Kleijn, 1990). These high correlations are consistent with expectations predicted by models of depression which focus on the long-term effects of 117 stress (Gold, Goodwin, & Chrousos, 1988), and may be most indicative of specific subtypes of depression in which anxiety is an integral factor in the development of the disorder (Weiss et. al, 1981). Correlates of anger, such as hostility and irritability, have been found to be highly linked to depression (Becker & Lesiak, 1977; Biaggio & Godwin, 1987). A closer analysis of the data shows that, in spite of the strong association between Hostility and Irritability (r = .85), their patterns of correlations show them to be distinct cOnstructs that have distinctive links to other affective experiences. In the current study, Irritability was associated more highly with Depression (1 = .97) than with Anxiety (1 = .78). In contrast, Hostility was linked similarly to both Depression (1; = .73) and Anxiety (I. = .76). Irritability was associated with greater distress across all factors except Mixed Anxiety; Mixed Anxiety correlated .73 with Irritability and .80 with Hostility. Quit The guilt items in the current study were not homogenous; two items had a distinctly different pattern of correlations than the other nine. An analysis of these items showed that the two distinct items were taken from the BDI. The instructions for this instrument ask the respondent to answer in terms of their experience over the past week. In contrast, the other items were taken from the Chang-Hunter Guilt scale, in which the items are depicted as general statements. In this way, it is likely that the first two guilt items depict a more transient, or state, measure of guilt, whereas the other nine depict a more enduring, or trait, measure of the construct. 118 In contrast to expectations that guilt would be more highly linked to depression than to anxiety, neither state nor trait Guilt distinguished well between Anxiety and Depression. In the current study, the factor describing symptoms of trait Guilt was correlated .30 with Pure Anxiety and .34 with Pure Depression. By contrast, state Guilt was correlated .69 with Pure Anxiety and .77 with Pure Depression. This large contrast is consistent with previous findings which have suggested that state guilt measures may not be good representatives of more general, or trait, guilt (Charles & Levine, 1995). Shame, Depression and Anxieg Many theorists have linked shame to depression. The current results support a stronger link between Shame and Pure Depression (r = .71) than Pure Anxiety (I = .50). However, it is noteworthy that what I have termed “mixed” symptoms of the constructs do not discriminate well; these correlations are .68 and .62 for Depression and Anxiety, respectively. Shame, therefore, may be linked to specific aspects of the depressive experience, or it may be that somatic symptoms are less relevant to the experience of shame than are more psychological experiences of distress. Anaclitic and Introjective Dimensions For the purposes of the current study, I selected a subsample of the items which Blatt listed as loading most strongly on each factor, which did not also load significantly on another factor. Using this method produced an “Anaclitic” scale which consists entirely of the items in the Dependency factor, consistent with Blatt’s (1974) depiction of the anaclitic dimension as “dependent.” However, it should be noted that the two clusters 119 which make up this factor in the current study are Dependency and Need for Approval. This is contrary to Blatt’s (1974) original conceptualization of these two styles, in which it is the introjective style which is linked to need for approval. In contrast to the homogeneity of the “Anaclitic” scale, the “Introjective” scale that I derived is more diverse. It is composed of all of the items in the Insecurity factor, which included the clusters Unstable, Insufficiency, and Insecure; plus Mo items from the Self-Critical Cluster; and four items which were difficult to classify and were not included in the content analysis. Two of the omitted items were considered to be too vague for inclusion in the content analysis, but were consistent with a self-critical theme (“There is a considerable difference between how I am now and how I would like to be” and “I tend not to be satisfied with what I have”). Two other omitted items were difficult to classify at all (“No matter how close a relationship between two people is, there is always a large amount of uncertainty and conflict” and “I tend not to be satisfied with what I have”). The heterogeneity of this scale is consistent with Blatt’s diffuse depiction of the introjective style as a mixture of self-dissatisfaction, ambivalence toward others, and failure to meet expectations. Because of the heterogeneity of the Introjective scale and the contamination of the Anaclitic scale with a predominant theme fi'om the original conceptualization of the introjective style, there are severe problems of construct validity in regard to these two scales. Researchers who are trying to understand relationships between dependency and self-criticism and other constructs would be advised to look carefully at the actual content 120 of these scales before attempting to use them as a measure of those constructs. Current Anxiety and Depression Scales We can also begin to evaluate the instruments from the study which are used to measure anxiety and depression. For example, are they overweighting relevant items or, perhaps weighting irrelevant items? To address this question, I looked more closely at the anxiety and depression measures utilized in the study to ascertain how well they sample the symptoms which appear to be most relevant to these constructs (See Table 20). A Content analysis of the BAI shows that two thirds of the items are linked to Pure Anxiety, and only one third are linked to Mixed Anxiety. That is, the BAI consists of 11 items from the Autonomic Hyperarousal cluster, 3 items associated with Motor Tension, 5 items associated with Fear, and 2 items associated with Vigilance. Perusal of the SCL—Anxiety scale shows that half of the items are characteristic of Pure Anxiety, and half are linked to Mixed Anxiety; this scale consists of 1 item from the Autonomic Hyperarousal cluster, 4 items fi'om Motor Tension, and 5 items associated with Fear. A content analysis of the BDI shows that 19% of the items are linked to Pure Depression; more specifically, there are 2 symptoms of Fatigue, 1 of Irritability, and l of Hopelessness. In addition, 33% of the items are linked to Mixed Depression; 2 of Dysphoric Mood and 4 of Anhedonia. Twenty-three percent of the items are linked to Severe Symptoms; 2 of Guilt, 2 of Suicidality, and 2 of Appetite Disturbance. The remaining 23% fall into other categories; 1 of Worry, 4 of Self-Criticism, and l of Sleep Disturbance. Number of Items from Each Cluster Represented in Anxiety and Depression Instruments. 12] Table 20 BA] SCL- BDI SCL- Anxiety Depression PURE Autonomic ANXIETY Hyperarousal l l 1 Motor Tension 3 4 MIXED ANXIETY Fear 5 S 2 Vigilance 2 MIXED DEPRESSION Dysphoric Mood 2 3 Anhedonia 4 3 Worry 1 PURE DEPRESSION Fatigue 2 2 Irritability 1 Hopelessness l l SEVERE SYMPTOMS Guilt 2 l Suicidality 2 l Appetite Disturbance 2 SHAME Self-Criticism 4 Selchsteem 1 Sleep RESIDUAL Disturbance 1 l 122 A content analysis of the SCL-Depression scale shows that the items in this scale are not as representative of the relevant symptoms as the items in the BDI. Only 23% of the items were linked to Pure Depression; more specifically, 2 of the items were symptoms of Fatigue and 1 of Hopelessness. In addition, 38% of the items were linked to Mixed Depression; 3 of Dysphoric Mood and 3 of Anhedonia. Fifteen percent were linked to Severe Symptoms; 1 of Guilt and 1 of Suicidality. The remaining 28% were linked to other categories; 1 of low Self-Esteem, l of Sleep Disturbance, and 2 of F ear. Examination of the clusters which define the anxiety and depression factors in the current study allows a more specific evaluation of how representative the scales are of the symptoms which appear to be most relevant. A content analysis suggests that the anxiety scales are more representative of the relevant syndrome than are the depression scales. Smptoms of Anxiety The present findings regarding symptom clusters associated with anxiety were consistent with previous factor analytic studies in which psychological and somatic clusters were found (Cloninger, 1988b; Buss, 1960). Notably, the symptom clusters of Motor Tension and Autonomic Hyperarousal, which I have termed “Pure Anxiety,” were consistent with what Cloninger (1986; 1988b) described as somatic anxiety, and the symptom clusters of Fear and Vigilance which I termed “Mixed Anxiety” were consistent with what Cloninger (1986; 1988b) described as cognitive anxiety. Somatic symptoms were not as highly linked to dependency as would have been 123 predicted by previous findings (Beck, Epstein, & Harrison, 1983). This link may be more likely to be found in clinical studies which are able to focus differentially on distinct subtypes of anxiety. Drug trials support the view that somatic and psychological symptoms of anxiety should be seen as distinct; Benzodiazapines, which affect a subsystem of the gamma- aminobutyric acid (GABA) system, were effective in treating somatic symptoms and in decreasing hypervigilance. In contrast, imipramine, which blocks reuptake of norepinephrine and serotonin affected psychological symptoms, such as anxiety, interpersonal sensitivity, anger-hostility, paranoid ideation, and obsessive compulsive symptoms (Hoehn-Saric, McLeod, & Zimmerli, 1988). Heterogeneig in the Construct of Depression The current data suggests that the construct of depression is quite heterogeneous. This is in striking contrast to the symptoms of anxiety, which were (1) more easily described using relatively few factors, and (2) better represented by items in existing instruments. Depression was represented most clearly by the symptoms of fatigue, hopelessness, and irritability. Symptoms of depression with higher correlations with anxiety included dysphoric mood and anhedonia, often considered to be th_e defining symptoms of depression. In addition, the cluster which I have termed “Severe Symptoms” was largely composed of symptoms associated with depression, such as guilt, appetite disturbance, and suicidality. Notably, the factor I termed “Alienation,” composed of items associated with rejection sensitivity, was also highly correlated with symptoms of Mixed 124 Depression and Severe Symptoms. These symptoms were not included as part of a larger mixed factor because of striking differences in their links to other constructs under study. Notably, Dependency was strongly linked to Alienation (r = .54), and not at all linked to Severe Symptoms (r = .07). The link between Dependency and Mixed Depression fell between these two extremes (r = .28). i It is notable that Dysphoric Mood, a symptom linked conceptually very strongly to depression, was not as strong a marker of this syndrome as were other symptoms. This finding is consistent with suggestions in the literature that depression may present, particularly in adolescence, without dysphoric mood as traditionally conceptualized (Nurcombe et. al., 1989). There are numerous indications in the literature that there are distinct subtypes of depression which may vary considerably in terms of predominant affective experience. For example, van Praag (1994) suggests the existence of a distinct subtype of depression characterized primarily by anxiety and/or aggression deregulation as , the primary symptoms, associated with diminished serotonergic metabolism. Within this syndrome, depressed mood is seen as a derivative, rather than a primary, symptom. Anxieg and Depression The correlation between Pure Anxiety and Pure Depression is a high .77. However, epidemiological studies typically find an asymmetry in the relationship between them. Patients who are first diagnosed with an anxiety disorder with no severe symptoms of depression are often later given a diagnosis of depression (Angst, Vollrath, Merikangas, & Ernst, 1990). On the other hand, patients who are first diagnosed as depressed without 125 severe symptoms of anxiety are rarely later diagnosed with anxiety disorders (Angst et al., 1990) Does the regression plot for Pure Anxiety and Pure Depression show any similar asymmetry? The contingency table for Pure Depression as a function of Pure Anxiety is shown in Table 21. Note that this table is constructed using scale scores which are not perfect measures of the two constructs. The error of measurement produces a blurring of position in this table. In particular, the columns show a larger variation than would be shown for a perfect measure of Pure Depression. Table 21 Contingency Table Relating Pure Depression and Pure Anxiety PURE ANXIETY P o 1 2 s 4 Total U R 5 1 1 E 4 6 1 1 1 9 D E 3 5 5 3 4 1 18 P R 2 34 26 10 1 71 E g 1 76 14 2 92 1 o o 122 4 126 N Total 237 55 16 6 3 317 126 Close examination of this table shows that as anxiety goes up, there is always an increase in depression. That is, this table is consistent with the findings that high levels of anxiety tend to lead to high levels of depression at a later point in time. On the other hand, consider the data for 0 anxiety. There is a considerable range for depression. The highest levels of depression are not seen for this level of anxiety, but very high levels are seen in the sample. Furthermore, the lack of observations of high depression must be considered in relation to the fact that the onset of depressive disorder tends to be later than onset for anxiety. In this college population, there are many students who will become severely depressed later in life. These may be the missing cases of extreme depression for the low anxiety group. Thus, there are cases of high depression even for people with no anxiety. This is the asymmetry. High anxiety always accompanies high depression, but high depression can occur without high anxiety. This asymmetry is consistent with research regarding anxiety driven depression. Because at the state level anxiety produces depression, if there is anxiety, then there will be depression (Barlow, 1991). In these cases, serotonergic drugs have been found to be therapeutic. However, many people do not respond to drugs which affect the serotonergic system, suggesting that these individuals are depressed for reasons other than anxiety (van Praag, 1994). Those individuals who are low in anxiety and high on depression are those who tend to report depression first. That group tends to remain stable over time. These findings are not consistent with the learned helplessness findings, and suggest that there is at least one type of depression other than that which is anxiety 127 driven. Conclusions as to the Measurement of Anxietmd Depressipp The results of this study in part support recent work (Watson et. al, 1995a; 1995b) which suggests that vigilance/arousal best distinguishes anxiety whereas irritability and psychomotor retardation best distinguish depression. The findings do not support the utility of invoking a general distress factor to explain differences between the two syndromes. Relatively high correlations do not tell the whole story; in spite of commonalities between the negative affects, this study points to clear and potentially meaningful distinctions between these symptoms, as well. Future research with clinical and older adult populations will help to elucidate whether the current findings will generalize beyond a non-clinical young adult sample. The literature would suggest that anxiety and depression should show greater difl’erentiation in clinical samples (Nurcombe et al., 1989) and greater comorbidity with increasing age (Brady & Kendall, 1992). Overall, the results of the analyses point to validation problems when measuring complex constructs. There were often no clear distinctions between state versus trait measure of the same construct, and many of the constructs were erroneously named, inadequately sampled, or confounded in widely-used instruments. In light of the variety of treatments available to address symptoms of mood disorders, including both anxiety and depressive syndromes, it becomes particularly important to distinguish clusters of symptoms which may have implications for treatment 128 choice. The severity and the seriousness of certain associated symptoms, such as suicidality, make it particularly important to clarify symptom-treatment links. At this point in time, there are numerous attempts to reconcile clinical understanding with neurophysiological research, and to make sense of conflicting findings regarding psychotherapeutic versus medical interventions (Schore, 1994). This type of integration of theory and research may help us to focus our understanding toward more efficient and effective intervention. APPENDIX A 601- 143. 151. 172. 177. 179. 142. 184. 188. 193. 195. 196. 602-MOTOR TENSION 130. 136. 146. 158. 131. 139. 175. 187. A SCL-ANX SCL-ANX SCL-ANX SCL-ANX SCL-ANX SCL-DEP BA1 BA1 BA1 BA1 BA1 SCL-SOM SCL-SOM SCL-SOM SCL-SOM SCL-ANX SCL-ANX SCL-ANX EA] 129 APPENDIX A ITEMS BY THEME - ALL SCALES Suddenly scared for no reason. Feeling fearful. Spells of terror or panic. The thought that something bad is going to happen to you. Thoughts and images of a fiightening nature. Feeling of being caught or trapped. Fear of the worst happening. Terrified. Fear of losing control. Fear of dying. Scared. Headaches. Pains in heart or chest. Pains in lower back. Soreness of your muscles. Nervousness or shakiness inside Trembling. Feeling so restless you couldn‘t sit still. Unsteady. 130 603-MOTOR TENSION (cont’d). 1 91 . BAI 1 92. BAI 603-AU'TONOMIC 132. SCL-SOM 1 56. SCL-SOM 159. SCL-SOM 160. SCL-SOM 162. SCL-SOM 1 55. SCL-ANX 180. BAI 1 81 . BAI 182. BAI 185. BAI 186. BAI 190. BAI l 94. BAI 197. BA] 1 98. BAI l 99. BAI 200. BAI 604-VIGILANCE 165. SCL-ANX 1 83. BAI Hands trembling. Shaky ERAROUSA Faintness or dizziness. Nausea or upset stomach. Trouble getting your breath. Hot or cold spells. A hunp in your throat. Heart pounding or racing. Numbness or tingling. Feeling hot. Wobbliness in legs. Dizzy or lightheaded. Heart pounding or racing. Feelings of choking. Difficulty breathing. Indigestion or discomfort in the abdomen. Faint. Face flushed. Sweating (not due to heat). Feeling tense or keyed up. Unable to relax. 131 604-VIGILANC E (cont’d) 1 89. BAI 605- ABILITY 1 35. SCL-HOST l 18. BDI 606-m SEQ QB“: X 35. INTRO] 37. INTRO] 607-UPSET 207. NEGAF F 229. NEGAFF 254. NEGAFF 263. NEGAFF 608-TENSE 210. NEGAFF 236. NEGAFF 257. NEGAFF 609-WO Y 149. SCL-DEP 128. BDI Nervous. Feeling easily annoyed or irritated. I don't get irritated at all by the things that used to irritate me. I never really feel secure in a close relationship. Often, I feel threatened by change. I sometimes get too upset by minor setbacks. I can get very upset when little things don't go my way. I don't get very upset when things go wrong. (-) Little things upset me too much. I sometimes feel ”on edge” all day. I would describe myself as a tense person. 1 often feel nervous and ”stressed." Worrying too much about things. I am so worried about my physical problems that I cannot think about anything else. 132 610-DYSPHORIC MOOD 140. SCL-DEP 147. SCL-DEP 1 17. BDI 108. BDI 148. SCL-DEP 61 l-FATIGUE l 37. SCL-DEP 1 7 l . SCL-DEP 1 22. BDI 124. BDI 612-ENERGY 202. ENERGY 205. ENERGY 226. ENERGY 238. ENERGY 245. ENERGY 248. ENERGY 253. ENERGY 256. ENERGY 260. ENERGY 274. ENERGY 279. ENERGY 288. ENERGY Crying easily. Feeling lonely. I used to be able to cry, but now I can't cry even when I want to. I am so sad or unhappy that I can't stand it. Feeling blue. Feeling low in energy or slowed down. Feeling everything is an effort. I can't do any work at all. I am too tired to do anything. I sometimes rush from one activity to another without pausing for rest. I lead an active life. Other people sometimes have trouble keeping up with the pace I set. I put a lot of energy in everything I do. I can work hard, and for a long time, without feeling tired. My pace is usually quick and lively. Most days I have a lot of "pep" or vigor. People would describe me as a pretty energetic person. In my life, 1 would rather try to do to much than too little. I am sometimes ”on the go" so much that I wear myself out. I have more energy than most people I know. People sometimes tell me to slow down and "take it easy." 133 6 3- NTHUSIASM 215. POSTEMP I get excited when I think about the future. 262. POSTEMP I get pretty excited when I'm starting a new project. 235. POSTEMP I am usually alert and attentive. 217. POSAFF People would describe me as a pretty enthusiastic person. 290. POSAFF I am usually pretty excited about the things that I do. 614-SLEEP DISTURBAN C E 123. BD1 1 wake up earlier than I used to and can't get back to sleep. 615-APPETITE DISTURBANCE 125. BDI I have no appetite at all any more. 126. BDl I have lost more than 15 pounds. 616-COGNITIVE DIFFICULTIES 244. NEGTEMP Sometimes life seems pretty confusing to me. 247. NEGTEMP I am sometimes troubled by thoughts or ideas that I can’t get out of my mind. 617-D PENDENCY 2. ANACL Without support from others who are close to me, I would be helpless. 9. AN ACL The lack of permanence in human relationships doesn't bother me. (-) 20. ANACL I would feel like I'd be losing an important part of myself if I lost a very close friend. 23. ANACL I often think about the danger of losing someone who is close to me. 38. ANACL Even if the person who is closest to me were to leave, I could still "go it alone." (-) 134 617-DEPENDENCY (cont’d) 50. ANACL 55. ANACL 65. ANACL 618-INSUFFICIENCY 1 1. INTRO] l6. INTRO] Ifsomeone I cared about became angry with me, I would feel threatened that he (she) might leave me. After an argument, I feel very lonely. Being alone doesn't bother me at all. Many times I feel helpless. There are times when I feel ”empty" inside. 619-NEED FOR APPROVAL 34. 45. 12. 26. 32. GZESELE-CRITICISM 1 10. 114. 115. 7. 121. 62. ANACL ANACL ANACL ANACL ANACL BDI BDI BDI INTRO] BDI INTRO] I frnd it very difficult to say "no” to the requests of friends. I wony a lot about hurting or offending someone who is close to me. I seldom worry about being criticized for things I have said or done. (-) I am not very concerned with how other people respond to me. (-) I constantly try, and very often go out of my way, to please or help people I am close to. I feel I am a complete failure as a person. I hate myself. I blame myself for everything bad that happens. I often find that I don't live up to my own ideals or standards. I believe that I look ugly. I am very satisfied with myself and my accomplishments. GZI-ANHEDONIA 133. SCL-DEP 1 50. SCL-DEP 1 1 l. BD1 1 19. BDI 129. BD1 1 20. BDI 622-SELF-ESTEEM 33. SELF -EFF 59. SELF-EFF 60. SELF-EFF 157. SCL-INTP 176. SCL-DEP 623-EFFICACY 1. SELFEFF 15 SELFEFF 24. SELFEFF 6 4-UNS B E 36. INTRO] 58. INTRO] 135 Loss of sexual interest or pleasure. Feeling no interest in things. I am dissatisfied or bored with everything. 1 have lost interest in other people. I have lost interest in sex completely. 1 can’t make decisions at all anymore. I have many inner resources (abilities, strengths). What I do and say has a very strong impact on those around me. I sometimes feel that I am “special.” Feeling inferior to others. (-) Feelings of worthlessness. (-) I set my personal goals and standards as high as possible. I feel I have many responsibilities 1 must meet. Other people have high expectations of me. The way I feel about myself frequently varies: There are times when I feel extremely good about myself and other times when I see only the bad in me and feel like a total failure. Very frequently, my feelings toward someone close to me vary: There are times when I feel completely angry, and other times when I feel all-loving towards that person. 136 635-STATE GUILT 1 12. BD1 I feel guilty all the time. 1 13. BD1 I feel I am being punished. 626-AN GER 214. NEGAF F My anger frequently gets the better of me. 233. NEGAFF I often take my anger out on those around me. 283. NEGAF F I sometimes feel angry for no good reason. 627-HOSTILITY 134. SCL-INTP Feeling critical of others. 144. SCL-HOST Temper outbursts that you could not control. 168. SCL-HOST Having urges to beat, injure, or harm someone. 169. SCL-HOST Having urges to break or smash things. 174. SCL-HOST Getting into frequent arguments. 178. SCL-HOST Shouting or throwmg things. 6Z§-NEGATIVE AFFECTIVITY 204. NEGAFF I often experience strong emotions such as anxiety or anger without knowing why. 281. NEGAF F Things seem to bother me less than most other people. (-) 629-POSITIVE AEEEgflgfljx 201. POSAF F C I have the ability to approach tasks in such a way that they become interesting or fun. 21 l. POSAFF C I lead a very interesting life. 223. POSAFF C In my life, interesting and exciting things happen every day. 137 629-POSTTIVE AFFECT IVITY (cont’d) 23o. POSAFFC 242. POSAFFC 286. POSAFFC W 109. BDI 163. SCL-DEP W 1 16 BDI 138. scuorzp 632-srer-c0Nscrous 167. SCL-INTP 17o. SCL-INTP 173. SCL-INTP gas-mummy 209. DISINHIB 216. DISINHIB 227. DlSINI—IIB 237. DISINI—IIB 249. DISINHIB 258. DISINHIB I live a very full life. It takes a lot to get me excited. (-) 1 often feel lively and cheerful for no particular reason. I feel that the future is hopeless and that things cannot improve. Feeling hopeless about the future. I would kill myself if I had the chance. Thoughts of ending your life. Feeling uneasy when people are watching you or talking about you. Feeling very self-conscious with others. Feeling uncomfortable about eating or drinking in public. I often stop in the middle of one activity to start another one. Before I make a decision I usually try to consider all sides of the issue. (~) The way I behave often gets me into trouble on the job, at home, or at school. I rely on careful reasoning when making up my mind. (-) I always try to be fully prepared before I begin working on anything. (-) I am not an "impulse buyer.” (-) 138 W (cont’d) 270. DISINHIB 273. DISINHIB 634- 261. DISINHIB 635-PERSISTENC E 222. DISINHIB *##(Also in 640)*** 636-P YFULNESS 218. POSAFF C 241 . POSAFF C 27 1 . POSAFF C 637- SOCIAL 2 l 9. DISINHIB 234. DISINHIB 250. DISINHIB 252. DISINHIB 267. DISINHIB 275. DISINHIB 278. DISINHIB 282. DISINHIB SPONSIBIL When I'm having a good time, 1 don't worry about the consequences. 1 am a cautious person. (-) I am a serious-minded person. (-) I work just hard enough to get by. People would describe me as a pretty enthusiastic person. 1 can make a game out of some things that others consider work. 1 often feel playful around other people. I believe in strictly playing by the rules. (-) I greatly dislike it when someone breaks accepted rules of good behavior. H I would not use others' weaknesses to my own advantage. (-) I really enjoy beating the system. Lying comes easily to me. I've done a lot of things for which I could have been (or was) arrested. When I decide things, I always refer to the basic rules of right and wrong. (-) I often get out of things by making up believable excuses. 139 637-ANTISOCIAL (cont’d) 284. DISINHIB 1 get the most fun out of things that others consider immoral or illegal. 285. DISINHIB ' 1 would never hurt other people just to get what 1 want. (-) 289. DISINHIB At times I've done some petty thievery. 638-SENSATION SEEKING 213. DISINHIB If 1 had to choose, 1 would prefer having to sit through a long concert of bad music to being in a bank during an armed robbery. (-) 225. DISINHIB I rarely, if ever, do anything reckless. (-) 228. DISINHIB 1 get a kick out of really scaring people. 231. DISINHIB IfI had to choose, I would prefer being in a flood to unloading a ton of newspapers from a truck. 240. DISINHIB 1 would much rather party than work. 277. DISINHIB I spend a good deal of my time just having fun. 287. DISINHIB I don't ever like to stay in one place for long. 264. DISINI-IHB I like to show-off. 269. POSAFF C I like to stir up some excitement when things are getting dull. Q39-DISORGANIZATION 280. DISINHIB Taking care of details is not my strong point. 640- AMBITIOUS 222. DISINHIB 1 work just hard enough to get by. (-) 255. DISINHIB I've been told that I work too hard. 140 641-ROSENBERG SELF-ESTEEM SCALE 67. SE 70. SE 73. SE 74. SE 80. SE 82. SE 84. SE 88. SE 91 . SE 93. SE 64 -MASTER 71 . MAST 76. MAST 78. MAST 79. MAST 81 . MAST 83. MAST 85. MAST 86. MAST 90. MAST 94. MAST On the whole, I am satisfied with myself. I feel that 1 have a number of good qualities. I feel I do not have much to be proud of. (-) I feel that I am a person of worth, at least on an equal plane with others. At times I think I am no good at all. (-) I am able to do things as well as most other people. I take a positive attitude toward myself. All in all, I am inclined to think I am a failure. (-) I certainly feel useless at times. (-) I wish 1 could have more respect for myself. (-) If 1 put my mind to it I can learn almost anything. I find life an endless series of problems with no end in sight. (-) Most of the time I think that the world is an exciting place to live in. My work in general is at least as good as the work of the guy next to me. When I decide to do something, I do it. I feel that I have no talent whatsoever. (-) I repeat things continuously to be sure that 1 am right. (-) When 1 want something, I just sit around wishing I could have it. (-) I feel that I am able to make decisions. I am fearful of growing up. (-) 141 643-COOK SHAME SCALE 97. 98. 99. 100. 101. 102. 103. 104. 105. 106. SHAME SHAME SHAME SHAME SHAME SHAME SHAME SHAME SHAME SHAME I feel like 1 am never quite good enough. I feel somehow left out. I think that people look down on me. Compared to other people I feel like I somehow never measure up. 1 scold myself and put myself down. I see myselfas being very small and insignificant. 1 say to myself, "how could anyone really love me or care about me?" I feel defective as a person, as if something is basically wrong with me. I feel intensely inadequate and full of self-doubt. I see myself striving for perfection only to continually fall short. 644-CHANG - HUNTER GUILT SCALE (Ifll’T GUILI) 68. 69. 72. 75. 77. 87. 89. 92. 95. GUILT GUILT GUILT GUILT GUILT GUILT GUILT GUILT GUILT 1 often cannot forgive myself for having caused deep pain in those I love or care for. I feel horrible for having hostile feelings toward other people. 1 have felt very guilty for letting down those close to me. It bothers me that I have not done more for my parents or family members. I have felt very guilty for not being there when someone close to me needed me. Sometimes I cannot forgive myself for how I have treated others. Sometimes I hurt people I love or care for and feel very guilty about it afterwards. When I let my anger out, I often feel very guilty afterwards. I often feel guilty for being better off than my family members. 142 645-IRRITABILITY (GTS) 220. NEGTEMP 259. NEGTEMP 646-WORRY (GTS) 212. NEGTEMP 239. NEGTEMP 272. NEGTEMP 647- FEAR (GTS) 221 . NEGTEMP 268. NEGTEMP Small annoyances often irritate me. I have days that I’m very irritable. I frequently find myself worrying about things. 1 often worry about things I have said or done. 1 wony too much about things that don’t really matter. Sometimes I will suddenly feel scared for no reason. I worry about terrible things that might happen. 648- SLEEP DISIQQANCE (GTS) 251 . NEGTEMP 649- ALIENATED 1 52. SCL-INTP 1 53. SCL-INTP 1 54. SCL-INTP RESIDUAL ITEMS 141 . SCL-INTP l 57. SCL-INTP 276. NEGTEMP 251 . NEGTEMP I often have difficulty sleeping because of my worries. Your feelings being easily hurt. Feeling others do not understand you or are unsympathetic. Feeling that people are unfiiendly or dislike you. Feeling shy or uneasy with the Opposite sex. Feeling inferior to others. Often life feels like a big struggle. I often have difficulty sleeping because of my worries. 143 W (cont’d) 265. NEGTEMP I am often troubled by guilt feelings. APPENDIX B 144 3. a. ... .... ... 2. 9. ..n on 2. 2. 8 2. 2 an an 25288. .2 8. a o. 2. 2. .... 8 3 mm «m .... x 3. 6 .~ on 22.8.25 2.2.58 .2 .n 8. a .m- ... 8 S 8. a em .m :. 2. u. 2. 8 8:35.29 658.2 .2 o. R 8. on. .N. 2. 8 we ... 2 2. 8 em .1... 3. 2. 8:25.25 ..8_m .... .... .m- 8. 8. 8 as 8. .2 2. 2. 3.. an. .m. R. em. 3.. .aa........... .2 2. ... .u. 8 8. 3. an. 2. n. a or «a. 2. :. .... 2. 183...”. .N. 9. 8 2. «a 3. 8. ... ... S .... x 2. 2. :. «a «a 2.2.: .: 9. 8 9. a... on. ... 8. 8. 2. a. an 2. 2. S 2. 2. 862 6.5.328 .2 x 8. we .2 mm- ... 8. 8. g 8 2. a. 8. 8 8 ea 2.53 .6 mm 6 2 2. n- S 2 E 8. 2 on ..n 2. on 2. 9 .85; ... mm 8 2 2. s 9. me we 2 8. 2. S 2 mm R 28. ea: .6 .m .m em 3. 9.. 3 mm 2 on 8 8. ..n 2. .m 2. R 2:82.. .6 4m .5 9. an. S. 2.. no a. ..m S ..m 8. 2. we 2. as 2.55.... .m a. 2. on .m. 2. 2. 2. .... 2. mm 2. 2. 8. we 2. ... 288: 8535 ... e 2 2. R. ..- .2. 2.... 8 on ..N .m 8 we 8. e6 8 Essaém 6.5222 .m .N 2 3. 8. 2- 2. ea 8 2. R ea 2. 2. ea 8. 6.. 8.2.6.2262 .m on 8 2n 3.. 2- 2. ..e e... a. an R 2 ... E 2. 8. 632. .82 .. 2 2 v. 2 a. : o. a m a e m e m N . ”Emmy 2335‘ 58:00 .3 3:on 8.8m =a do.“ .632 5222.50 NN 03am. 145 16' II. VN av cv N. N. a0 on an we ow On or Z. N@ Mb ac 3052285923 .mm m N _a 0N am. a 7 av mo 2. _N N X.” No hv 50 av E. £3225 .Nm em 2. 3. 9.... ”N. cm 3 va on an we 3 we 8 mm. ca 3053—232 . _ m w 7 ON. N. .a ma 0v. 3.. on- _N- NN. 00. MV. Nm. ~N. hm- mm. 492.83 2.28m .2” mm 5N N be. mm- 3 No .a N@ no. 5 a0 we Nm av 2e. eb§80b< 0339.2 .aN N _ uh Nv an. NN. am 3 ma em 3. me an no wk. as on .9532 .mN Nv :4 2 ”N. 5.. am 94 On mm mm 3 mm mm mm :4 3 5.092. .hN hN oo— om 0N. m T Na no a N 2 av 3. av no 2. as. :50 .oN 2 w. v C. N. m— a C E N_ a— NN o- N v N. 98:22an .N av n- w. _ m. N. Nv mv en an Nm Na an 3. ON N am 03839 .N b an. 3. ”N ov a. w. m- o_ v w m- N_ N _ . a. N. 5359:an .MN @ 7 av. mm- av mv ow. N? M». NM- mm- an- no. mvt 3.- No. 5. Eooamm..=om .NN mm am g 3. N. mm 3. a on 2.” 0m E. an 8 Nb or «Eaves—2 . _ N am ow 3 on- am. Nb on va 9.. mm 2. mm cm 3 No m5 3:20.23 .oN hv N 3 3. NT cm 3 mm av am 3— we mm VN ov «V _c>oaa< 3.“ .302 .a_ G N ”N mm. 5N. S we as 8 mm 5 on. G $4 co 8 5:22.685 .3 2 m— 3 m— N— 2 o— a w h o n v m N _ . -....- NEWER. .9283 2 63a... 146 vv vM FM aN- ON. Mb Na 5— OM NM NM Ma ah MM MN. 0M coca5=< .av MN cN vM Z. _ 7 NM NM MM MM 5M cv NM MM NN vN hN .02—3565 moo—M .Mv MM 0M MN vM- M _- _v vM_ MM 2. MM MM oM NM MM Mv 2- 13m .vv 2. vN N— M7 :- hv Mv an Mn. MM _M vM MM aN ov ov 20:03 .Mv MM MT v M- v vM MM bN vs 2: v FM hM v— M- M_ .b:58t.: .Mv vv CN N. a. v. NM _M . av MM 5M _M Mv NM MN MM MM €3.52 d 93:9 =50 .vv 5M Mv cM NM. NM. 5M 5M Mb hv Mv MM MM OM 5M cM MM 0.008 082M .Mv MN. NM- “M. «M cM Mv. Mv. MM- _v. MN- MM. _M- MM- ~M- aM. Mv- Cobov @232 .Nv bN- MM- MN. vv Mv _M- av. MM- Nv- MM. aM- bM- NM- MM. sv- MM- 3.32805 Eoofim-hom ..v MN 5 _. a MN- MM- MN 2 M_ M _ MN MN N T M- ~ _ SOME—5‘ .ov N M_ 2 MT 2. N_ N. ON M. o a v N a M 2 335985 .aM o— b v. M v— a. 2. MT 2 M M M M- N- v. N. Mcfioom cocoa—3M .MM — 5M N— Mv. MM. 2 M a M- N a M 2. 2 M. v— _a_ooacc< NM 7 MN. M _- Na Mh Mv. Mv. ov- aN- a _ - MM. MN- M T _N- aN- MM- 332% .MM M. M v _ 7 v7 M _ T 2. MT M- v N- _ M- N. 0365395 .MM a MM v— Mv. _N- a— 3 N N_ v- OM _N M 2 a. vN 3.22:5 .vM M _ M ~ 3 M— N— _ _ o— a M h M M v M N — mzmmh .5253 mm 23- 147 8 a. a. 8 mm 8 2 m. 8 8 a- 2 9 8 E 8. 55823 .2 a 8 a- 8 a. a. R 2 8 b 2- 8 8 S 8 3. 3:855 3880 .2 a 2. on- R a. :. 8. a- m. 8- 8. 8 8 8 R 2 8:35.39 289?. .2 8m 8 a. «a a. 2 8 v a 3. mm. s. z. z 8 z 85925 8%. .2 8- 8- a S. 8. 8. on. 2- a- 8 8 8. 8. 3. mm. 2. .23358 .2 2- 8- 3 mm. «a. w 2. a- 8. 8 8 8m. 8- 2. R. w .328 .2 a. 8 8. 8 8 an a. 2 S a. 8. 8 up 8 8 8 2.2.: .: 8 3 3.- 8 8 8 8 a 8 a. N9 2. 2 ... 8 8 802 0885 .2 8 a 8. a no 8 8 2 8 n. m. a 3 8 2. 8 $53 a a 8 a. S x 8 8N : 8. 2 mm. 8 8 8 8 8 .35 .w 8" 2 a. S. a. 2 w. 2 8. v mm. 2 mm on 8. 3 .025 s 3. 8 9w 8 8 8 8 2 a a 8- 8 fl 8. S 2. 238.5 .8 8 8 a. 8 8 8 z. «N a w. 8. 8 8 8 2 8 .8ng .n c. 8 an- 8 8 mm a. 2 S 2 8. 8 8 8 8 8 A35: 85.35 .v 8 8 a. 8 8 8 8 N 8 2- 8. 8 3. a S 2 .aBEém 252.23 .... 8 8 R. 8 2 z. z. e 8 .... «v Q. 8 8 8 um :22: as: .m z. 8 mm- a 8 3. 8 2 8 u- xx 2. m. 8 8 8 6an 5m ._ 8 a 8 on 8 R 8" mm x mm a a on 2 2 2 88.5 9:53 8 29¢ 148 6v No pm. No 3 av mm 3 an N. no. 3 3 we 3 E 8053285928 .mm 8— Nb an. 3. 3 NV ow _ _ S on- no. on _ no _N on o b=anSm .Nm Nb 2: an- Vm mw hv Mb 3 av n 7 «c. on 5 3. . no we 80280—082 Am mm. an- 2: cv- mm. 3... mm- m _. hm- mm an 9»... rm. 5 T N... a. .b_>=oo.€< Eggnog .om we 3 ow. 2: 3 co on S on m. .m. 3 8 no 2. we .b_>:oo.c< 3.3502 .am 3 mo mm- Va 8. oo 8 fl mm n... No. 2. no 94. mv mm £533 .3 NV 5v 3. ca 8 2: on mm on an. no. mm mm an 3 cm Low—2 KN ow Mb mm. an 00 an 8. 2 mm a T he. an cw MN 3 2 =30 6N Z 2 m7 C m_ «N N. 8. o- 3. mm. a mm m. 2 m— «cocoafihom .mu 5 av hm- ow an on mm 2 8. m- an. .m em 3 3 an 03883 .3 mm. m 7 mm m. n 7 mm- m _. .m- m- 2: E. 0.. a T R 2 NM xoaomcmfiom .MN 8. no. 3 3- no. no. no. mm. mm. X. 8. hw. mu. m T _n- _N. Evofiméom .NN on on on. 3. I. an on a Tm o 7 so. o2 g 2 mm m— £5352 . _N we hm rm. 8 3 nm em mm on 3. mm. E. 2: on on Na. .32t0..=om .8 —N 3. 2- «w on mm mm 2 3 pm m. _- m— an 8. 3 on 3553‘ 8.. v82 .2 om no me. 2. mv 3 vv 2 no N. 3. Na or 3 2: E. 55.0535 .3 Nm :m on a mu 5N 3 mm vn mm mm a. on 2 3 S NEE 3.253 mm 23. 149 8 2. 8- 8 8 8 8 2 8 2 8. :- 8 8 8 8 88.22 .8 2 8 2. 8 2 8 8 m 8 2- 8- 8 8 2 8 8 .8555 2.88 .8 8 8 8- a 8 2. 8 8 8 8. 2w 8 8 8 8 8 .8". .2. 8 8 8- 8 8 8 8 z a m 8. 8 2. 8 8 8 .253 .2. m- 8 2. 8 8 2. 2.. 2 R : 2. n 8 8 8 8 .5352— .8 8 8 2- 8 8 8 8 2 8 m 8. 8 8 8 8 :- 35: a. 2.2.2 86 .8 8 8 8- : 8 2. 8 8 R 8. 2.. 8 8 8 2. 8 382258 .8 21 2v 8 21 2.. 8. R- 8. 21 8 8 8- 2.- 8. 8- 8- 828 522 .2. a. 8. 8 8- 8. G- 8- 8. 8. 8 8 8- ;- 8- 27 8- 228.802 8882.8 .8 _. 8 8. 2 o : v 2. 8 8- 8. n 8 8 2 8 328:2 .8 a 2 2- 8 2 2 a a 2 2. 2. 8 2 8. 2 2 88.985 .8 n n 2 a. 2 8 n 8 2 :- o _- 2. a 2. o. 2588 2.2.38.9. .8 2 .- 8. 2 8 8 o 8 _ 8. 8- 2 2 2.- 2 o 388:5. .8 8- 8. 2. 8. 8. 8. 8- :- 2- 8 8 8- 8. 2 8. - 52: .8. 2 2. - w. v N a 2 2 8. - o a 2- m. N 0.22.8822 .8 8 :- 8. 8 8 8 2 8 2 8. 8. 8 :- 2 8 2 828.5 .8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 2 2 2 2228 .5282 8 228 150 8 8 8 8 8 2.. 8 8- 8. 8 2 a. 22 8. a 2 28 588225 2 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8- 8- 8 8 2 2 2- 8. a 8 8022282225 322288 .2 8 8 8 8 2- 8 8 8- 8- 2- 2 p 8 8- 8 8 8 8:35:20 32222.? .2 8 8 8 2 v 2 8 2m- 8- a 22 2.. 2 2- 2. 2 8 82.22.25 .288 .2 8- 22- 8.. 2- 8. a. 8- 8 8 8- 2- a 2x 8 22- 8. 8- .5823225 .2. 8. 22. 2. 22- 2. 2.. 8- 8 8 8- 22- v2 8. 2. :- 8- 8- 18.22 .2 2. 8 22. 2. 8 8 .8 8. 28. 8 2 a. 2 8. m 2 8 0822.2 .22 8 8 8 8 8 2m 8 8. 2. 2 2. 2- 22 21 2 2.2 8 8822 0.8222229 .2 S2 8 8 2. 8 2- 8 8. 3 2 8 2- a 21 2- 8 8 .803 .8 8 8 2. 8 8 8 2... 22.. 2a m 2 2 8. 8- 2- 2 8 .8228 .8 8 8 8 8 82 8 2. 8- 8- 2 22 8 8 2- 2- 2.2 2. 28.2: .8 8 8 8 28 2.. 28 8 8. 8. 8 2.. 8 o 8- m- 8 8 238:2 .8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 28- 8- 8 .- 22 22 8- .. 8 8 .2833 .8 2. 8 8 8 8 8 8 8- 8- 8 n 8. 2- 2- 8- 8 :- 2882 8522225 .2. 8 8 8 8 2 8 8 2m. 8- 2- a N- 2 8- 2 2 8 2322239922 02:52.23. .8 2. 8 8 8 2 8 8 8- 2.. m- 8 2.. 2 8. m- 2 2. .5238 as: .8 8 8 8 8 2 8 8 8. 8- 22 22 N. 2 8- 2. 8 8 636 .82 .2 8 8 2. 8 8 8 8 2. :- 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8 8,288 2252.82 8 02,28 151 w». on 8, an an R S 21 x. cm 2 w. a a. o. 2 8. ”8558883 .2 8 z 3 a m. on S 2.. a. _- a m 2 mm- _ cm 2 $.35 an on on. 3. 3- 8 mm 3 3 $- 2 2 n v 2.. b :- S $058202 .2 on. 2. mm- on. 2. 2- on. S 2. an. 2. 2 mm. a: u. m". R. .2385 02:82 .8 2. on a 2 2 2‘ g N? 2.- 2 o a. 2 2”. x. mm 8 .2585 2,522 .8 8 2 2. mm 3 mm mm 2.. 2.. a 2 2 a an- v «M 3 £58: .2 2- : 2 G 2 mm 2- 2.. a. : 2 R R on. m 2. o.- ..omé .R 2 an S 2- h- 2. a a... x- v a m m 2. a 2 2 =50 .3 2 m on 2 2 2 on «N. a. 2.. a on a :- 2 8 z .85-Eon .3 2‘ cm :- 3 R 3 a 8. on. a 2 2 _ :- 2 2 mm 2.325 .3 _ 2- «a- m : m on- 2‘ mm mm- 2- :- 21 2 mm. 3.. N- 5823.8 .2 $- 2. 8. mm. 2. a. 2.. 2. 3 mm. 2- c on- on N. R. 8. 88.-meow .NN : a 8 R m 3. 3. an- 2- m on _. 2 2- o mm 3 288.3- ..N 8 3 2 2. a 2. 2 2- 5- 3. 2 \- 2 an. m z. 2. 32.92% .8 an 2 :- 2. a on 2- R- mm- mm 9 m 2.. 2 2. 2 8 ESE-«8.. ..82 .2 8 mm 2 a 3. 2 :- 3- $- 2 2 _- 2 a. m. cm 2. 2822.235 .2 2- 2- :- 2. 2- 3 2‘ S S 8 an an R on 2 3 an El 3.282 mm 032- 152 .2 -.R R 2., mm on on on. 2.. 2 o o 2 mm- m. 2 3 8.385.. .2. R 8. 3 2. 2 R R 8. R- e o ... 2 ._ 2 2 2 on 3:35.30 28a .2. R 3 c2 3 2. 2. 2 m2 8. 2 2 «.- o R.- N 3 8 18.-2 .2. 2 2. z. 8. 2. 2. «m 3.. 2.. a m _. 2 .R. :- : an 1an .2. mm mm 2. 2. 8. mm R 2- R. u 7 : R. : 2- v R .2.st .2. an o“ 2. 2. mm 2: : .... 2.. 2 : 2 .- 2 m R R 35:38:03.5 .3. on R 2 2 am 3 8. R. 2. R 2 a 2 2.. R a 3 38805.5 .2. on- on. 2 32 2- .... R- 8. 8 R. R- ...- 2 G ...- :1 2.. 2..-299.82 .2. 2.. R- o2 2.. R. 2.. m2 8 8. .2 R- 2 2- 2 :- :.. ..2 gxcaoé 58.2.23 .:. 2 o 2 o R 2 R R- R. :2 v on x a. on 2. on 32.35.. .2. o ... 2 m 2. : 2 R- R. v 8. .. on R. 2 .m 2 82:28.5 .3 o ... m- _- : 2 a m. 2 on .. .2 .0 mm R 2. 2 2.28m 8:35 an 2 2 o 2 R- .- 2 2 2- 3 cm S .2 mm- 8 2. x 382.5. .R R- 2.- .R. .R. : 2 2.. G 2 a. on. mm mm- 2: _ 2- 2m- .22: .3 m- 2 R :- 2- m R :- :- on 2 R on _ 8. 2. 2 0.22885 .2 2 2 x : v R R 3.. :.. 2. R 2. 2. 2- 2. 8. 2 222.5 ...m 2. 2. S 2. 2. 3. 2. 9 :. 2. an an R 3. mm ..m mm 8222. .9283 mm uBu-H REFERENCES REFERENCES Abramson, L. Y., Metalsky, G. 1., & Alloy, L. B. (1989). Hopelessness depression: A theory-based subtype of depression. Echological Review. $0), 358-372. Akiskal, H. S. (1994). Dysthymia: Clinical and external validity. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, §_9_(Suppl. 383), 19-23. Akiskal, H. S., & Weise, R. S. (1992). The clinical spectrum of so-called “minor” depressions. American Jourgal of Psychotherapy, 313(1), 9-22. Arnerican Psychiatric Association (1987). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (Third ed., rev). Washington DC: American Psychiatric Association. Andrews, G., Stewart, 6., Allen, R., & Henderson, A. S. (1990). The genetics of six neurotic disorders: A twin study. Journ_al of Affective Disorders. _1_9_, 23-29. Angst, J ., Vollrath, M., Merikangas, K. R, & Ernst, C. (1990). Comorbidity of anxiety and depression in the Zurich cohort study of young adults. In J. D. Maser & C. R. Cloninger (Eds), Comorbidig of mood and anxiety disorders (pp. 123-137). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press. Apter, A., van Praag, H. M., Plutchik, R, Sevy, S., Kom, M., & Brown, S. L. (1990). Interrelationships among anxiety, aggression, impulsivity, and mood: A serotonergically linked cluster? Psychiatry Research, 3_2_, 191-199. Arieti, S., & Bemporad, J. R. (1980). The psychological organization of depression. American Journal of Psychiatg, lfl, 1360-1365. Bagby, R. M., Cox, B. J., Schuller, D. R., Levitt, A. J., Swinson, R. P., & Joffe, R. T. (1992). Diagnostic specificity of the dependent and self-critical personality dimensions in major depression. Journal of Affective Disorders ;_6_, 59-64. B'axlow, D. H. (1985). The dimensions of anxiety disorders. In A. H. Tuma & J. D. Maser (Eds), Anxieg and the anxiety disorders (pp. 497-500). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Bartlett, E. S., & Izard, C. E. (1972). A dimensional and discrete emotions investigation of the subjective experience of emotion. In C. E. Izard (Ed), Patterns of emotions: A new analysis of anxiety and depression (pp. 129-175). New York: Academic Press. 153 154 Beck, A. T. (1967). Depression: Clinical. experimental_and theoretical aspects. New York: Holber. Beck, A. T., & Beamesderfer, A. (1974). Amount of depression: The Depression Inventory. In P. Pichot (Ed), Psychological megurements in psychopharmacology (pp. 151-169). Basel: Karger. Beck, A. T., Brown, 6., Eidelson, J. I., Steer, R. A., & Riskind, J. H. (1987). Differentiating anxiety and depression: A test of the cognitive content-specificity hypothesis. Jorml of Abnormal Psychology, 26(3), 169-183. Beck, A. T., & Clark, D. A. (1988). Anxiety and depression: An information processing perspective. In R. Schwarzer & R. A. Wicklund (Eds), Anxieg and self-focused attention (pp.4l-54). Chur, FRG: Harwood. Beck, A. T., Epstein, N., Brown, G., & Steer, R. A. (1988). An inventory for measuring clinical anxiety: Psychometric properties. Journal of Consulting and Clinigrl Psychology, i616), 893-897. Beck, A. T., Epstein, N., & Harrison, R. (1983). Cognitions, attitudes and personality dimensions in depression. British Journi of Cpgnitive Ther_py, 1, 1-16. Beck, A. T., Ward, C. H., Mendelson, M., Mock, J ., & Erbaugh, J. (1961). An inventory for measuring depression. Archives of General Psychiatg, 1;, 57-62. Becker, E. W., & Lesiak, W. J. (1977). Feelings of hostility and personal control as related to depression. Journal of Clinical chhology, 33(3), 654-657. Biaggio, M. K., & Godwin, W. H. (1987). Relation of depression to anger and hostility constructs. Psychological Reports, fl, 87-90. Blaney, P. H. (1977). Contemporary theories of depression: Critique and comparison. Journal of Abnormaflsycholcgy, §6_(3), 203-223. Blatt, S. J. (1974). Levels of object representation in anaclitic and introjective depression. Psychoanalyaic Study of the Child, 12, 107-157. Blatt, S. J ., D'Aflliti, J. P., & Quinlan, D. M. (1976a). Experiences of depression in normal young adults. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, QM), 383-389. Blatt, S. J ., D'Afiliti, J. P., & Quinlan, D. M. (1976b). Depressive Experiences Questionnaire, New Haven, CN: Yale University. 155 Blatt, S. J., Hart, 8., Quinlan, D. M., Leadbetter, B., & Auerbach, J. (1993). Interpersonal and self-critical dysphoria and behavior problems in adolescents. Journal of Youth m Adolescence. 2_2(3), 253-269. Blatt, S. J ., Quinlan, D. M., & Chevron, E. S. (1990). Empirical investigations of a psychoanalytic theory of depression. In J. Masling (Ed), Empirical studies of psychoanalflic theories (Vol. 3, pp. 89-147). Hillsdale, NJ: Analytic Press. Blatt, S. J., Quinlan, D. M., Chevron, E. S., McDonald, C., & Zuroff, D. (1982). Dependency and self-criticism: Psychological dimensions of depression. Journal of Consultinwd Clinical Psychology, $0), 113-124. Blatt, S. J ., & Zuroff, D. (1992). Interpersonal relatedness and self-definition: Two prototypes for depression. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology Review. 1;, 527-562. Block, J ., & Robins, R. W. (1993). A longitudinal study of consistency and change in self-esteem from early adolescence to early adulthood. Child Development, Q4, 909-923. Brady, E. U., & Kendall, P. C. (1992). Comorbidity of anxiety and depression in children and adolescents. Psychological Bulletin, m0), 244-255. Breier, A, Albus, M., Pickar, 1)., Zahn, 'r. P., Wolkowitz, o. M., & Paul, s. M. (1987). Controllable and uncontrollable stress in humans: Alterations in mood, neuroendocrine and psychophysiologic function. American Jgumal of Psychiatg, L43, 1419-1425. BI’eier, A., Chamey, D. S., & Heninger, G. R. (1985). The diagnostic validity of anxiety disorders and their relationship to depressive illness. American 101M Psychiatg, 1_42(7), 787-797. B1‘eslau, N., Davis, G. C., & Prabucki, K. (1987). Searching for evidence on the validity of generalized anxiety disorder: Psychopathology in children of anxious mothers. Psychiatry Research, m, 285-297. Bl‘etherton, I. (1985). Attachment theory: Retrospect and prospect. In I. Bretherton & E. Waters (Eds), Growing points of attachment theory and research. Monogpaphs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 59(1-2; Serial No. 209), 3-35. Bl‘ewin, C. R., & Fumham, A. (1987). Dependency, self-criticism and depressive attributional style. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 2Q, 225-226. 156 Brophy, C. J., Norvell, N. K., & Kiluk, D. J. (1988). An examination of the factor structure and convergent and discriminant validity of the SCL-9OR in an outpatient clinic population. Journal of Personality Assessment. 2(2), 334-340. Brown, J. D., & Silberschatz, G. (1989). Dependency, self-criticism, and depressive attributional style. Journal of Abnoml Psychology, 28(2), 187-188. Buss, A. H. (1960). Two anxiety factors in psychiatric patients. Journal of Abnormal Sociology, 2_3, 56. Carey, G., & DiLalla, D. L. (1994). Personality and psychopathology: Genetic perspectives. Jourfll of Abnormal Psychology, $0), 32-43. Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1991). A control-process perspective on anxiety. In R Schwarzer & R. A. Wicklund (Eds), Anxiety and self-focused altention (pp. 3-8). Chur, FRG: Harwood Academic Publishers. Chang, S. S. (1988). The measurement of shame and its relationship to other personality Mtg. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Michigan State University. Charles, M., & Levine, R. (1995). Measuring guilt and shame: Construct vaJigigpan_d Lemma» with associated variables. Manuscript in progress. Michigan State University, East Lansing, MI. Chrousos, G. P., & Gold, P. W. (1992). The concepts of stress and stress system disorders: Overview of physical and behavioral homeostasis. WAC. American Medical Association 267 1244-1252. Clark, D. A. & Beck, A. T. (1989). Cognitive theory and therapy of anxiety and depression. In P. C. Kendall & D. Watson (Eds), Anxiety and depression: Distinctive and overlapping features (pp. 379-411). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Clark, D. A., Beck, A. T., & Stewart, B. (1990). Cognitive specificity and positive-negative affectivity: Complementary or contradictory views on anxiety and depression? Journal of Abnormal Psycholpgy, 22(2), 148-155. Clark, L. A. (1989). The anxiety and depressive disorders: Descriptive psychopathology and differential diagnosis. In P. C. Kendall & D. Watson (Eds), Anxing Ed depression: Distinctive Mverlapping features (pp. 83-129). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. 157 Clark, L. A., & Watson, D. (1989). The General Temperament Survey. Unpublished manuscript, Southern Methodist University, Dallas, TX. Clark, L. A., Watson, D., & Mineka, S. (1994). Temperament, personality, and the mood and anxiety disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, EU), 103-116. Clifl‘ord, C. A., Hopper, J. L., Fulker, D. W., & Murray, R. M. (1984). A genetic and environmental analysis of a twin family study of alcohol use, anxiety, and depression. Genetic Epidemiology, 1, 63-79. C loninger, C. R., (1986). A unified biosocial theory of personality and its role in the development of anxiety states. Psychiatric Developments, 2, 167-226. Cloninger, C. R. (1987). A systematic method for clinical description and classification of personality variants: A proposal. Archives of General Psychiatg, fl, 573-588. Cloninger, C. R. (1988a). A unified biosocial theory of personality and its role in the development of anxiety states: A reply to commentaries. Psychiatric Davelopments, 2, 83-120. Cloninger, C. R. (1988b). Anxiety and theories of emotion. In R. Noyes, Jr., M. Roth, & G. D. Burrows, (Eds), findboci of anxiety. Vol. 2: Classification. etiological factors and associated disturbjances. Amsterdam: Elsevier. Cloninger, c. R., Martin, R. L, Guze, s. B, & Clayton, P. J. (1981). A blind follow-up and family study of anxiety neuroses: Preliminary analysis of St. Louis 500. In D. F. Klein, & J. Rabkin (Eds), Anxiety: New reseflh and changing concepts (pp. 137-150). New York: Raven. Cloninger, C. R., Martin, R. L., Guze, S. B., & Clayton, P. J. (1990). The empirical structure of psychiatric comorbidity and its theoretical significance. In J. D. Maser & C. R. Cloninger (Eds), Comprbidig pf mood and anxiety disorders (pp. 439-462). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press. Cook, D. R. (1985). The Intemalized Shame Scale. Unpublished manuscript, University of Wisconsin, Menomonie. C08m, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1985). Th; NEQ Personality Inventogy manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Services. C()sta, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1987). Neuroticism, somatic Complaints, and disease: Is the bark worse than the bite? Joamal of Parsonality, fl, 299-316. 158 Crowe, R. R., Noyes, R., Pauls, D. L., & Slymen, D. S. (1983). A family study of panic disorder. Archives of GenerarlPsychim, fl, 1065-1069. Cyr, J. J ., McKenna-Foley, J. M., & Peacock, E. (1985). Factor structure of the SCL- 90R: Is there one? Journal of Personality Assess_ment. fi(6), 571-578. Deakin, J. F. W., Guimaraes, F. S., Wang, M., & Hensman, R. (1991). Experimental tests of the 5-HT receptor imbalance theory of affective disturbance. In M. Sandler, A. Coppen, & S. Hamett (Eds), S-Hydromgptamine in psychiatu: A apactrum of ideas (pp. 143-156). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Depue, R. A., Luciana, M., Arbisi, P., Collins, P., & Leon, A. (1994). Dopamine and the structure of personality: Relation of agonist-induced dopamine activity to positive emotionality. Journal of Personalig and Social Psychology, QB), 485-498. Derogatis, L. R., Lipman, R. S., & Covy, L. (1973). The SCL-90: An outpatient psychiatric rating scale. Psychopharmacology Bulletin, 2, 13-28. Dohrenwend, B. P. (1990). Notes on some epidemiologic studies of comorbidity. In J. D. Maser & C. R. Cloninger (Eds), Comorbidity of mood and anxiety disorders (pp. 177-185). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press. Drugan, R. C., Maier, s. F., Skolnick, R, Paul, s. M., & Crawley, J. N. (1985). An axiogenic benzodiazapine receptor ligand induces learned helplessness. European Journal of Pharmacology, m, 453-457. I)‘Jbovsky, S. L. (1993). Approaches to developing new anxiolytics and antidepressants. Journal of Clinical Psychiatu, 5_4(5, Suppl), 75-82. Iiril(sson, E., & Humble, M. (1990). Serotonin in psychiatric pathophysiology: A review of data from experimental and clinical research. In R. Pohl & S. Gershon (Eds), The biplogical basis of psychiatric treatment. In Progress in basic clinical pharmacology (Vol. 3, pp. 66-119). Basel: Karger. Ettenberg, A. (1989). Dopamine, neuroleptics and reinforced behavior. Neuroscience and Biobehayioral Reviews. 1_3_, 105-111. Eysenck, H. J ., & Eysenck, S. B. G. (1968). Eysenck Personalng Qpestionnaire manual. San Diego, CA: Educational and Industrial Testing Service. Fllhr, S. K., & Shean, G. (1992). Subtypes of depression, efficacy, and the Depressive Experiences Questionnaire. The qumal of Pachology, _1_2§(5), 495-506. 159 Garber, J ., Miller, W. R., & Seaman, S. F. (1979). Learned helplessness, stress, and the depressive disorders. In R. A. Depue (Ed), The psychobiology of the depressive disorders: Implications for the effects of stress (pp. 335-363). New York: Academic Press. Gibbons, F. X. (1991). Self-evaluation and self-perception: The role of attention in the experience of anxiety. In R. Schwarzer & R. A. Wicklund (Eds), Anxiety and self-focused attention (pp. 15-25). Chur, FRG: Harwood Academic Publishers. Gold, P., W., Goodwin, F. K., & Chrousos, G. P. (1988). Clinical and biochemical manifestations of depression: Relations to the neurobiology of stress: II. New England Journal of Medicine. 319(7), 413-420. Colin, 8., & Hartz, M. A. (1979). A factor analysis of the Beck Depression Inventory in a mildly depressed population. Journg of Clinical Pachology, 25, 308-319. Gotlib, I. H, & Cane, D. B. (1989). Self-report assessment of depression and anxiety. In P. C. Kendall & D. Watson (Eds), Anxiety and depress_ion: Distinctive an_d overlapping features (pp. 131-169). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Gotlib, I. H., & Meyer, J. P. (1986). Factor analysis of the Multiple Afl‘ect Adjective Checklist: A separation of positive and negative affect. Journal of Personalig and Social Pachology, fl, 1161-1165. Green, D. P., Goldman, S. L., & Salovey, P. (1993). Measurement errors mask bipolarity in afi‘ect ratings. Jouma1 of Personality and Social Psychology, fi(6), 1029-1041. Grinker, R. R., Miller, J ., Sabshin, M., Nunn, R., & Nunnally, J. (1961). The phenomena of depressions. New York: Hoeber. Hague“, 0., & Grasbeck, A. (1990). Comorbidity of anxiety and depression in the Lundby 25-year prospective study: The pattern of subsequent episodes. In J. D. Maser & C. R. Cloninger (Eds), Comorbidig of mgod and anxieg disorders (pp. 139-152). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press. Henderson, N. D. (1986). Human behavior genetics. Annual Review of Psychology, 22, 403-440. Heninger, G. R., & Chamey, D. S. (1988). Monoamine receptor systems and anxiety disorders. Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 11(2), 309-326. Hirschfield, R. M. A., Klerman, G. L., Andreasen, N. C., Clayton, P. J ., & Keller, M. B. 160 (1986). Psycho-social predictors of chronicity in depressed patients. British Journal of Psychiatg, 1g, 648-654. Hirschfield, R. M. A., Klerman, G. L., Lavori, P., Keller, M. B., Griffith, P., & Coryell, W. (1989). Premorbid personality assessments of first onset of major depression._ Archives of General Psychiatty, @, 345-350. Hoehn-Saric, R., McLeod, D. R., & Zimmerli, W. D. (1988). Differential effects of alprazolam and imipramine in generalized anxiety disorder: Somatic versus : psychic symptoms. Journal of Clinical Psychiatgg, 42(8), 293-301. Insel, T. R., (1991). Serotonin in obsessive-compulsive disorder: A causal connection or more monomania about a major monoamine? In M. Sandler, A. Coppen, & S. Harnett (Eds), S-Hydromtyptamine in psychiatty: A spectrum of ideas (pp. 228- 257). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Izard, C. E., Blumberg, S. H., & Oyster, C. (1985). Age and sex differences in the pattern of emotions in child anxiety and depression. In J. T. Spence & C. E. Izard (Eds), Motivatioa, emotion, and personality. Amsterdam: North-Holland. Jardine, R., Martin, N. G., & Henderson, A. S. (1984). Genetic covariation between neuroticism and the symptoms of anxiety and depression. Genetic Epidemiology, 1, 89-107. Joseph, R. (1988). The right cerebral hemisphere: Emotion, music, visual-spatial skills, body-image, dreams, and awareness. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, fl, 630-673. Kashani, J. H., Beck, N. C., Hoeper, E. W., Fallahi, C., Corcoran, C. M., McAllister, J. A., Rosenberg, T. K., & Reid, J. C. (1987a). Psychiatric disorders in a community sample of adolescents. American Journal of Psychiatry, 133, 585-589. Kashani, J. H., Carlson, G. A., Beck, N. C., Hoeper, E. W., Corcoran, C. M., McAllister, J. A., Fallahi, C., Rosenberg, T. K., & Reid, J. C.. (1987b). Depressron, depressive symptoms, and depressed mood among a community sample of adolescents. American Journal of Psychiatty, 114(7), 931-934. Katz, M. M., Koslow, S. H., Berman, N, Secunda, S, Maas, J. W., Casper, R., Kocsis, J ., & Stokes, P. (1987). Multivantaged approach to the. measurement of behavioral and affect states for clinical and psychobiological research. Ps cholo 'cal Re orts, 55, 619-671. Katz, M. M., Maas, J. W., Frazer, A., Koslow, S. H., Bowden, C. L., Berman, N., Swarm, 161 A. C., & Stokes, P. E. (1994). Drug-induced actions on brain neurotransmitter systems and changes in the behaviors and emotions of depressed patients. Neuropsychopharrnacology, 3(2), 89-100. Katz, M. M., Robins, E., Croughan, J ., Secunda, S., & Swann, A. (1987).Behavioural measurement and drug response characteristics of unipolar and bipolar depression. Psychological Medicine, Q, 25-36. Kelly, D., & Walter, C. J. S. (1969). A clinical and physiological relationship between anxiety and depression. Britis_h Journal of Psychiatg, 1Q, 401-406. Kendall, P. C ., & Ingram, R. E. (1989). Cognitive-behavioral perspectives: Theory and research on depression and anxiety. In P. C. Kendall & D. Watson (Eds), Anxiety and depressipn: Distinctive and overlapping features (pp. 27-53). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. King, R., Margraf, J ., Ehlers, A., & Maddock, R. (1986). Panic disorder; Overlap with symptoms of somatization disorder. In 1. Hand & H.-U. Wittchen (Eds), Panic and phobias: Empirical evidence of theoretical models and longterm effects of behavioral treaanents (pp. 72-77). Berlin: Springer-Verlag. Klein, D. N., Harding, K., Taylor, E. B., & Dickstein, S. (1988). Dependency and self- criticism in depression: Evaluation in a clinical population. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, fl(4), 300-404. Kobak, R. R., & Sceery, A. (1988). Attachment in late adolescence: Working models, affect regulation, and representations of self and others. Child Develmpment. 52, 135-146. Kobak, R. R., Sudler, N., & Gamble, W. (1992). Attachment and depressive symptoms during adolescence: A developmental pathways analysis. Development and Psychopathology, 2, 461-474. Kraemer, G. W., Ebert, M. H., Schmidt, D. E., & McKinney, W. T. (1991). Strangers in a strange land: A psychobiological study of infant monkeys before and after separation from real or inanimate mothers. Child Develo ment, 6_2_, 548-566. Last, C. G., Francis, G., Hersen, M., Kazdin, A. E., Strauss, C. C. (1987). Separation anxiety and school phobia: A comparison using DSM-III criteria. Amancan Journal prsychiatty, 14:1, 653-657. Leckman, J. F., Weissman, M. M., Merikangas, K. R., Pauls, D. L., & Prusoff, B. A. 162 (1983). Panic disorder increases risk of major depression, alcoholism, panic, and phobic disorders in affectively ill families. Archives of General Psychiatg, fl, 1055-1060. Lips, H. M., & Ng, M. (1985). Use of the Beck Depression Inventory with three nonclinical populations. Canadian Journal of Ewioural Science. 18, 62-74. Lipsey, M. W., & Wilson, D. B. (1993). The efficacy of psychological, educational, and behavioral treatment. American Psychologist, £02), 1181-1209. Leonard, B. E. (1994). Serotonin receptors- Where are the going? International Clinical Psychopharmacology, 2(Suppl. 1), 7-17. Lopez-Ibor, J. J ., Jr. (1990). The spectrum of mood disorders. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 21 (9; Suppl), 62-64. Maier, S. F ., & Seligman, M. E. P. (1976). Learned helplessness: Theory and evidence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, fl, 3-46. Maier, W., Buller, R., & Hallmayer, J. (1988). Comorbidity for panic disorder and major depression: Results from a family study. In 1. Hand & H.-U. Wittchen (Eds), Panic and phobias: Treatments and variables affecting course and outcome (pp. 180-185). Berlin: Springer-Verlag. Martin, N. G., Jardine, R., Andrews, G., & Heath, A. C. (1988). Anxiety disorders and neuroticism: Are there genetic factors specific to panic? Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, fl, 698-706. McCranie, E. W., & Bass, J. D. (1984). Childhood family antecedents of dependency and self-criticism: Implications for depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, $(1), 3'8- Meltzer, H. Y. (1990). Role of serotonin in depression. In P. M. Whitaker-Azmith & S. J. Peroutka (Eds), Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences: The neuropharrnacology of serotonin (Vol. 600, pp. 486-499). New York: The New York Academy of Sciences. Merikangas, K. R. (1990). Comorbidity for anxiety and depression: Review of family and genetic studies. In J. D. Maser & C. R. Cloninger (Eds), Comorbidig of mood and anxith disorders (pp. 331-348). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press. 163 Mook, J ., van der Ploeg, H. M., & Kleijn, W. C. (1990). Anxiety, anger and depression: Relationships at the trait level. Anxiety Research, 2, 17-31. Mountjoy, C. Q., & Roth, M. (1982). Studies in the relationship between depressive disorders and anxiety states: Part 1. Rating scales. Journal of Affective Disorders, 4, 127-147. Mullaney, J. (1987). Measurement of anxiety and depression. In G. Racagni & E. Smeraldi (Eds), Anxious depression: Assessment and treatment (pp. 7-19). New York: Raven Press. Murray, L., & Trevarthen, C. (1985). Emotional regulation of interactions between two- month-olds and their mothers. In T. M. Field & N. A. Fox (Eds), Social perception in infants (pp. 177-197). Norwood, NJ: Ablex. Noyes, R., Clarkson, C., Crowe, R. R., Yates, W. R., & McChesney, C. M. (1987). A family study of generalized anxiety disorder. Ameriaan Journal of Psychiatpy, m, 1019-1024. Nurcombe, B., Seifer, R., Scioli, A., Tramonta, M. G., Grapentine, W. L., & Beauchesne, H. C. (1988). Is major depressive disorder in adolescence a distinct diagnostic entity? Jouraal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatiy, 2_8(3), 333-342. Ofl‘er, D., Ostrov, E., & Howard, K. (1989). The Offer Self-Ima e estionnaire for Adolescents. Chicago: Michael Reese Hospital. Ollendick, T. H., & Yule, W. (1990). Depression in British and American children and its relation to anxiety and fear. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 3(1), 126-129. Paul, S. M. (1988). Anxiety and depression: A common neurobiological substrate? Journal of Clinical Psychiatty, 42(10, Suppl), 13-16. Persons, J. B. (1986). The advantages of studying psychological phenomena rather than psychiatric diagnoses. American Psyghologist, £01), 1252-1260. Persons, J. B., Burns, D. D., Perloff, J. M., & Miranda, J. (1993). Relationships between symptoms of depression and anxiety and dysfunctional beliefs about achievement and attachment. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 192(4), 518-524. Petty. F., Kramer, G., & Moeller, M. (1994). Does learned helplessness induction by 164 haloperidol involve serotonin mediation? Pharmacology, Biochemistty, and Behavior, £6), 671-676. Pilkonis, P. A. (1988). Personality prototypes among depressives: Themes of dependency and autonomy. Journal of Personality Disorders. 2, 144-152. Prusoff, B. A., & Klerman, G. L. (1974). Differentiating depressed from anxious neurotic outpatients. Archives of GeneraiPsychiath, 3, 302-309. Puig-Antich, J ., & Rabinovich, H. Relationship between affective and anxiety disorders in childhood. In R. Gittelman (Ed), Anxing disorders of childhood. New York: Guilford. Regier, D. A., Burke, J. D., & Burke, K. C. (1990). Comorbidity of affective and anxiety disorders in the NHVIH Epidemiological Catchment Area program. In J. D. Maser & C. R. Cloninger (Eds), Comorbidityaof mood and anxietva disorders (pp. 113- 122). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press. Reich, J ., Noyes, R., Hirschfield, R., Coryell, W., & O'Gorman, T. (1987). State and personality in depressed and panic patients. American Journal of Psychiatg, l4_4(2), 181-187. Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-imags. Princeton: Princeton University Press. Sarason, I. G. (1985). Cognitive processes, anxiety and the treatment of anxiety disorders. In A. H. Tuma & J. Maser (Eds), Anxieg and the anxing disorders (pp. 932-946). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Schatzberg, A. F., Samson, J. A., Rothschild, A. J ., Luciana, M. M., Bruno, R. F ., & Bond, T. C. (1990). Depression secondary to anxiety: Findings from the McLean Hospital Depression Research Facility. Psychiatric Clinics of North America, QM), 633-649. Schmale, A. H., & Engel, G. L. (1975). The role of conservation-withdrawal in depressive reactions. In E. J .Anthony & T. Benedek (Eds), Depression and human existence. Boston: Little, Brown. Schore, A. N. (1994). Affect regalationana the origin of the self: The neurobiology of emotional development. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Schutz, R. A., Schutz, M. T. B., Orsingher, O. A., & Izquierdo, I. (1979). Brain 165 dopamine and noradrenaline levels in rats submitted to four different aversive tasks. Psychopharmacology 63 289-292. ,—9 Schwab, J. J ., Bialow, M. R., & Holzer, C. E. (1967). A comparison of rating scales for depression. Journal of Clinical Psycholggy, 2a, 225-226. Schwalbe, M. L. (1991). The autogenesis of the self. Joumad of the theory of social behavior, 21 , 269-295. Shapiro, J. P. (1988). Relationships between dimensions of depressive experience and evaluative beliefs about people in general. Personality and Sociafl’sychology Bulletin, 14(2), 388-400. Shaw, B. F., Vallis, T. M., & McCabe, S. B. (1985). The assessment of the severity and symptom patterns in depression. In E. E. Beckharn & W. R. Leber (Eds), Handbook of depres_sion: Treatment. assessment. and reseam (pp. 372-407). Homewood, IL: Dorsey Press. Shutty, M. S., DeGood, D. E., & Schwartz, D. P. (1986). Psychological dimensions of distress in chronic pain patients: a factor analytic study of Symptom Checklist-90 responses. Journal of Con_sulting and Clinical Psychology, 5_4(6), 836-842. Smith, T. W., O’Keeffe, J. L., & Jenkins, M. (1988). Dependency and self-criticism: Correlates of depression or moderators of the effects of stressfiil events? Journal of Personalig Disorders, 2(2), 160-169. Soubrie, P. (1986). Reconciling the role of central serotonin neurons in human and animal behavior. The Behavipral and Brain Sciences, 2, 319-364. Spielberger, C. D. (1972). Anxiety as an emotional state. In C. D. Spielberger (Ed), Anxieg: Current trends in theogg and research (Vol. 1; pp. 23-49). New York: Academic Press. Svrakic, D. M., Przybeck, T. R., & Cloninger, C. R. (1992). Mood states and personality traits. Journal of Affective Disorders, 2_4, 217-226. Swerdlow, N. R., & Koob, G. F. (1987). Dopamine, schizophrenia, mania, and depression: Toward a unified hypothesis of cortico-striato-pallido-thalamic fiinction. Behavioral and brain sciences, m, 197-245. Tellegen, A. (1985). Structure of mood and personality and their relevance to assessing anxiety, with an emphasis on self-report. In A. H. Tuma & J. Maser (Eds), 166 Anxiety and the anxiety disorders (pp. pp. 681-706). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Tellegen, A. (in press). The Multidimensional Personality Questionnaire. Minneapolis MN: University of Minnesota Press. Torgerson, S. (1985). Hereditary differentiation of anxiety and affective disorders. Britis_h Jourratl of Psychiatty, m, 530-534. Torgersen, S. (1990). A twin study perspective of the comorbidity of anxiety and depression. In J. D. Maser & C. R. Cloninger (Eds), Comorbidity of mood and anxiety disorders (pp. 367-3 78). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press. Uhde, T. W., Roy-Byme, P. P., Vittone, B. J ., Boulenger, J.-P., Post, R. M. (1985). Phenomenology and neurobiology of panic disorder. In A. H. Tuma & J. Maser (Eds), Anxiety and the anxiety disorders (pp. 557-566). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. van Praag, H. M. (1980a). Central monoamine metabolism in depressions: I. Serotonin and related compounds. Comprehensive Psychiatgz, 2_1(1), 30-43. van Praag, H. M. (1980b). Central monoamine metabolism in depressions: II. Catecholamines and related compounds. Comprehensive Psychiatg, £0), 44-54. van Praag, H. M. (1994). 5-HT-related, anxiety- and/or aggression-driven depression. International Clinical Psychopharmacology, 2(Supp1. 1), 5-6. van Praag, H. M., Asnis, G. M., Kahn, R. S., Brown, S. L., Korn, M., Harkavy Friedman, J. M., & Wetzler, S. (1990). Monoamines and abnormal behavior: A multiaminergic perspective. British Journal of Psychiatgg, 1_51, 723-734. van Praag, H. M., Asnis, G. M., Kahn, R. S., Brown, S. L., Korn, M., Harkavy Friedman, J. M., & Wetzler, S. (1990). Nosological tunnel vision in biological psychiatry: A plea for a functional psychopathology. In P. M. Whitaker-Amnith & S. J. Peroutka (Eds), Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences: The neurophamracology of serotonin (Vol. 600, pp. 501-510). New York: The New York Academy of Sciences. van Praag, H. M., Kahn, R. S., Asnis, G. M., Wetzler, S., Brown, S. L., Bleich, A., & Korn, M. L. (1988). Beyond nosology in biological psychiatry. S-HT disturbances in mood, aggression, and anxiety disorders. In M. Briley & G. Fillion (Eds), New concepts in depression (pp. 96-119). Basingstoke, England: Macmillan. 167 Vredenburg, K., Krames, L., & Flett, G. L. (1985). Reexamining the Beck Depression Inventory: The long and short of it. Psychological Reports, as, 767-778. Watson, D. (1988). Intraindividual and interindividual analyses of positive and negative affect: Their relation to health complaints, perceived stress, and daily activities. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 24, 1020-1030. Watson, D., & Clark, L. A. (1984). Negative affectivity: The disposition to experience aversive emotional states. chhological Bulletin, 26, 465-490. Watson, D., & Clark, L. A. (1992). On traits and temperament: General and specific factors of emotional experience and their relation to the five-factor model. Journal of Personality, @(2), 441-476. Watson, D., & Clark, L. A. (in press). Extraversion and its positive emotional core. Chapter to appear in S. Briggs, R. Hogan, & W. Jones (Eds), mam; Personalig Psychology. San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Watson, D., & Clark, L. A. (1993). Behavioral disinhibition versus constraint: A dispositional perspective. In D. M. Wegner & J. W. Pennebaker (Eds), Handbook of mental control. New York: Prentice-Hall. Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Carey, G. (1988). Positive and negative affect and their relation to anxiety and depressive disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, _91, 346-353. Watson, D., Clark, L. A., Weber, K., Assenheimer, J. S., Strauss, M. E., & McCormick, R. A. (1995a). Testing a tripartite model: 1. Evaluating the convergent and discriminant validity of anxiety and depression symptom scales. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 194(1), 3-14. Watson, D., Clark, L. A., Weber, K., Assenheimer, J. S., Strauss, M. E., & McCormick, R. A. (1995b). Testing a tripartite model: II. Exploring the symptom structure of anxiety and depression in student, adult, and patient samples. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 124(1), 15-25. Weiss, J. M, & Simson, P. G. (1985). Neurochemical mechanisms underlying stress- induced depression. In T. M. Field, P. M. McCabe, & N. Schneiderman (Eds), Stress and goping (pp. 93-116). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Weiss, J. M., Goodman, P. A., Losito, B. G., Corrigan, S., Charry, J. M., & Bailey, W. H. (1981). Behavioral depression produced by an uncontrollable stressor: 168 Relationship to norepinephrine, dopamine, and serotonin levels in various regions of rat brain. Brain Research Review. 2(2), 167-205. Weissman, M. M. (1990). Evidence for comorbidity of anxiety and depression: Family and genetic studies of children. In J. D. Maser & C. R. Cloninger (Eds), Comorbidity of mood disorders (pp. 349-365). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press. Weissman, M. M., Leckman, J. F ., Merikangas, K. R., Gammon, G. D., & Prusoff, B. A. (1984). Depression and anxiety disorders in parents and children: Results fi'om the Yale family study. Archives of General Psychiatg, 41, 841-852. Weissman, M. M., K. R., Gammon, G. D., John, K., Merikangas, Warner, V., Prusoff, B. A., & Sholomskas, D. (1987). Children of depressed parents. Archives of General Psychiatg, 44, 847-853. Zung, W. W. (1972). How normal is depression? Psychosomatics, 13, 174-178. Zuckerman, M., Persky, H., Eckman, K., & Hopkins, T. R. (1967). A multi-trait multimethod measurement approach to the to the traits (or states) of anxiety, depression, and hostility. Journal of Projective Techniques and Personalig Assessment, 21 , 39-48. Zung, W. W. (1972). How normal is depression? Psychosomatics, 1_3_, 174-178. Zuroff, D. C., Igreja, I., & Mongrain, M. (1990). Dysfunctional attitudes, dependency, and self-criticism as predictors of depressive mood states: A 12-month longitudinal study. Cogm'tive Therapy and-Research, fl(3), 315-326. Zuroff, D. C., Moskowitz, D. S., Wielgus, M. S., Powers, T. A., & Franko, D. L. (1983). Construct validation of the dependency and self-criticism scales of the Depressive Experiences Questionnaire. Journal pf Research in Perspnality, 12, 226-241. Zuroff, D. C., & Mongrain, M. (1987). Dependency and self-criticism: Vulnerability factors for depressive affective states. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, $0), 14-22. nrcuran sran UNIV. LreRnRrEs lllllllllllllllllllllllllllllllllllllllllllll11111111111111 31293014172542