, . TTTTTTTTTTTTI ( \CW‘ 2) LIBRARY Michigan State University THE EFFECTS OF PSYCHIATRIC SYMPTOMATOLOGY ON INVOLVEMENT AND BENEFIT FROM SELF-HELP IN A DUALLY DIAGNOSED SAMPLE presented by LISA C. JORDAN has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for PH.D. PSYCHOLOGY-URBAN AFFAIRS degree in Major professor WILLIAM s. DAVIDSON, 11 Date JUNE 10, 1996 MS U is an Affirmative Action/Equal Opportunity Institution 0-12771 PLACE IN RETURN BOX to remove this checkout from your record. TO AVOID FINES return on or before date due. r DATE DUE MTE DUE DATE DUE W W 1/98 chIRCJD‘DuopGS—p“ THE EFFECTS OF PSYCHIATRIC SYMPTOMATOLOGY ON INVOLVEMENT AND BENEFIT FROM SELF-HELP IN A DUALLY DIAGNOSED SAMPLE By Lisa C. Jordan A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Psychology and Urban Studies 1996 ABSTRACT THE EFFECTS OF PSYCHIATRIC SYMPTOMATOLOGY ON INVOLVEMENT AND BENEFIT FROM SELF-HELP IN A DUALLY DIAGNOSED SAMPLE BY Lisa C. Jordan This study was designed to investigate the efficacy of Alcoholics and Narcotics Anonymous (AA/NA) programs for persons with dual diagnoses of substance abuse and mental illness, using correlational analyses and E08 structural equation modeling with data from a group of 474 persons with dual diagnosis. All of the participants in this study received treatment at a public psychiatric hospital, and many were subsequently referred to AA/NA for sobriety support services. The current analyses were conducted with data from the hospital admissions interview, and four follow-up interviews, concluding at 14-months post-hospitalization. The results indicated that from hospital discharge through 10-months post-hospitalization, participants in this sample were attending AAINA at rates equivalent to those reported in studies of persons with singly diagnosed substance abuse disorders. These findings suggest that dual diagnosis is not necessarily a deterrent to AA/NA participation. However, participants with the most severe and chronic psychiatric problems were less likely to attend AA/NA, and were more likely to drop out if they did attend. Results from the structural equation models did not support the expectation of differential outcomes from AA/NA and other service use based on psychiatric symptomatology and chronicity. Persons who attended AAINA regularly had fewer alcohol and drug problems, and fewer psychiatric problems at 10- and 14-months post-discharge, regardless of prior symptomatology and chronicity. While AAINA participation was not directly related to psychological or family/social problems, or rehospitalization, reduction in alcohol and drug problems seemed to mediate the relationship between AAINA service use and functioning in these other domains. Use of outpatient mental health services alone, or in combination with AAINA was not significantly related to better outcomes. Participants with more severe and chronic psychiatric problems were more likely to continue using mental health services after hospitalization, although these services were generally not related to positive outcomes. Copyright by Lisa C. Jordan 1996 To John Henry Jordan, Sr. the person I admire most in this world. Thanks for you love and support. Your Baby Girl has become a doctor! ACKNOWLEDGMENTS Special thanks goes to my chairperson, William Davidson for all his encouragement and support during this project. Whenever I said “no I can’t,” he said “sure you can.” Thanks to him I am able to finish this project in a timely manner. I also want to thank the other members of my committee: Timothy Bynum, Robert Caldwell, Sandra Herman, Maxie Jackson, and Bertram Stoffelmayr. Each made a significant contribution to this work. Their contributions ranged from giving me creative ideas and input, providing me with intellectual stimulation, and helping me with data analysis to providing me with financial support to keep me from starving to death while I was in graduate school. I give thanks to my family and friends for always believing in me and giving me the inspiration to keep going through rough times. To Aunt Tina, my second mom, thanks for always being in my comer. To Uncle Frank, thanks for introducing me to Buddhism. Many hours of daimoku are reflected in this work. To my Buddhist family: Gita, Rosemary, Von, Keora, Maria, and others, thanks for chanting with me through my various crises. Special thanks to Vemita Marsh, my sister-friend, and sometimes mentor. Thanks for all your support and guidance, and your ever present listening ear. I couldn’t have done it without you. vi TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES .................................................................................................. ix LIST OF FIGURES ................................................................................................ xi CHAPTER 1 INTRODUCTION ................................................................................................... 1 Purpose of this Study .................................................................................. 6 REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE ............ 8 Description of the Literature Search ........................................................... 11 Factors Related to ANNA Outcome .......................................................... 11 Individual Characteristics and AA Attendance ................................. 14 Outcomes when AA is Aftercare to Professional Treatment ............ 16 Levels of Involvement and Outcome ............................................... 19 Summary of Literature on AA Involvement and Outcome ................ 20 Treating Persons with Psychiatric Disorders .............................................. 22 Psychiatric Symptomatology and Interpersonal Functioning ............ 24 Psychiatric Symptomatology and Involvement in Group Therapy ..... 26 Summary ........................................................................................ 32 Limitations of Prior Studies on AA .............................................................. 33 Goals of the Present Study ........................................................................ 34 STATEMENT OF THE PROBLEM ....................................................................... 41 HYPOTHESES .................................................................................................... 43 CHAPTER 2 METHOD ................................................................................................... . .......... 46 Sample ...................................................................................................... 46 Measures .................................................................................................. 50 Individual Characteristics & Prior Experiences ........................................... 52 Aftercare Treatment .................................................................................. 54 Outcome Measures ................................................................................... 57 Procedures ................................................................................................ 58 vii CHAPTER 3 RESULTS ............................................................................................................ 60 Data Analysis Strategy .............................................................................. 60 Missing Data Analyses .................................................................... '. ......... 62 Descriptive Statistics on Study Variable ..................................................... 66 Correlations among the Study Variables .................................................... 70 Hypotheses l & ll: Relationship of Individual Characteristics to AAINA Attendance ..................................................................................... 72 Hypotheses III & IV: Relationship of AAINA and Other Service Use to Functioning ..................................................................................... 75 Summary of Preliminary Analyses & Hypotheses Tests ............................. 80 The Structural Equation Models ................................................................. 81 Path Analysis Results ................................................................................ 85 Summary of Research Questions and Findings ......................................... 98 CHAPTER 4 DISCUSSION ..................................................................................................... 100 Relationship between Individual Characteristics and Service Use ............ 101 Relationship of Service Use to Outcomes ................................................ 103 Findings and Implications of the Mediated AAINA Treatment Model ........ 106 Limitations of the Current Study ............................................................... 107 Conclusions & Implications ...................................................................... 109 Future Directions ............................................................................. ‘ ........ 111 LIST OF REFERENCES .................................................................................... 112 APPENDIX ......................................................................................................... 123 viii LIST OF TABLES Table 1. Demographic Characteristics of Participants ............................... . .......... 48 Table 2. Distribution of DSM-lll-R Diagnoses ..................................................... 49 Table 3. Measures Used in Current Study .......................................................... 51 Table 4. Comparison among Participants with Complete vs. Missing Data .......... 64 Table 5. Descriptives on AAINA and other Service Use during Follow-up ........... 68 Table 6. Descriptives on Outcome Measures ..................................................... 69 Table 7. Correlations between Predictor Variables, Covariates, and Outcome Variables .............................................................................................. 71 Table 8. Logistic Regression of Symptom/Chronicity and Covariates on AAINA Ever ..................................................................................................... 74 Table 9. ANCOVA Predicting AAINA Attendance Pattern from Symptom/ Chronicity Groups and Covariates ........................................................ 76 Table 10. MANOVAs Predicting Functioning from AAINA Attendance Pattern ..... 77 Table 11. MANOVAs Predicting Functioning from Type of Services Used at Time 3 .................................................................................................. 78 Table 12. Path Analysis Results: Integrated Model (A) with # of Days Attending AAINA at Time 3 .................................................................................. 86 Table 13. Path Analysis Results: Integrated Model (A) with AAINA Attendance Pattern ................................................................................................. 87 Table 14. Path Analysis Results: Integrated Model (A) with Type of Service Use Time 3 ................................................................................................... 88 Table 15. Path Analysis Results: AAINA Treatment Model (B) with # of Days Attending AAINA at Time 3 .................................................................... 91 Table 16. Path Analysis Results: AAINA Treatment Model (A) with AAINA Attendance Pattern ................................................................................ 92 Table 17. Path Analysis Results: AAINA Treatment Model (A) with Type of Service Use Time 3 ............................................................................................ 93 Figure 1. Figure 2. Figure 3. Figure 4. Figure 5. Figure 6. APPENDIX Figure A-1. Figure A-2. Figure A-3. Figure A-4. Figure A-5. Figure A-6. Figure A-7. LIST OF FIGURES Model A - The Integrated Treatment Model ....................................... 36 Model B - The AAINA Treatment Model ............................................ 37 Measurement Model A1 .................................................................... 82 Measurement Model B1 .................................................................... 83 Significant Path Predicting Psychological Functioning ....................... 96 Significant Path Predicting Family/Social Functioning ....................... 97 Integrated Model: Number of Days Attending AAINA and Alcohol Problems ....................................................................................... 123 Integrated Model: Number of Days Attending AAINA and Drug Problems ............................................................................... p ........ 124 Integrated Model: Number of Days Attending AAINA and Psychological Problems .................................................................. 125 Integrated Model: Number of Days Attending AAINA and Family/Social Problems .................................................................. 126 Integrated Model: Number of Days Attending AAINA and Rehospitalization ............................................................................ 127 Integrated Model: AAINA Attendance Pattern and Alcohol Problems ....................................................................................... 128 Integrated Model: AAINA Attendance Pattern and Drug Problems ....................................................................................... 129 xi Figure A-8. Figure A-9. Figure A-10. Figure A-11. Figure A-12. Figure A-13. Figure A-14. Figure A-15. Figure A-16. Figure A-17. Figure A-18. Figure A-19. Figure A-20. Figure A-21. Figure A-22. Figure A-23. Integrated Model: AAINA Attendance Pattern and Psychological Problems .................................................................. 130 Integrated Model: AAINA Attendance Pattern and Family/Social Problems .................................................................. 131 Integrated Model: AAINA Attendance Pattern and Rehospitalization ............................................................................ 132 Integrated Model: Type of Service Use and Alcohol Problems ........ 133 Integrated Model: Type of Service Use and Drug Problems ............ 134 Integrated Model: Type of Service Use and Psychological Problems ........................................................................................ 135 Integrated Model: Type of Service Use and Family/Social Problems ........................................................................................ 136 Integrated Model: Type of Service Use and Rehospitalization ........ 137 AAINA Treatment Model: Number of Days Attending AAINA and Psychological Problems ................................................................. 138 AAINA Treatment Model: Number of Days Attending AAINA and Family/Social Problems .................................................................. 139 AAINA Treatment Model: Number of Days Attending AAINA and Rehospitalization ............................................................................ 140 AAINA Treatment Model: AAINA Attendance Pattern and Psychological Problems .................................................................. 141 AAINA Treatment Model: AAINA Attendance Pattern and F amily/Social Problems .................................................................. 142 AAINA Treatment Model: AAINA Attendance Pattern and Rehospitalization ............................................................................ 143 AAINA Treatment Model: Type of Service Use and Psychological Problems ........................................................................................ 144 AAINA Treatment Model: Type of Service Use and Family/Social Problems ........................................................................................ 145 xii Figure A-24. AAINA Treatment Model: Type of Service Use and Rehospitalization ............................................................................ 146 xiii CHAPTER 1 INTRODUCTION Self-help or Mutual Aid for serious problems such as substance abuse has been increasing rapidly over the past decade, both in terms of the number and types of groups and the people being served by them. Since the 19705, Alcoholics Anonymous (AA) membership increased by 7% annually, with the United States and Canada accounting for the largest percent of this increase (Alcoholics Anonymous, 1989). The number of AA chapters worldwide has increased more than 200% over the past ten years, with current estimates of more than 80,000 groups in 150 countries (Miller & McCrady, 1993). A number of reasons have been cited for this tremendous growth, including lack of extended family and community supports (especially in urban areas), increasing rates of mental health problems such as depression and addiction, and inaccessibility of professional services for low income and uninsured persons (Borman, 1992). For serious problems such as alcohol and drug addiction, there are often not enough treatment options available. AA is the largest substance abuse treatment program currently in use, and is the only treatment ever received by a majority of those who receive any help (Bradley, 1988). When persons with substance abuse 2 problems do receive residential treatment (typically for 28 days or less), maintenance of treatment effects is often compromised by inadequate aftercare and/or lack of community supports. This is probably one reason why referral to Alcoholics and Narcotics Anonymous (AA and NA) for aftercare support has become a common practice in professional treatment programs. Self-help groups are often the only source of continued help for persons who have begun the process of recovery. Another reason for the growth in AA groups is that many persons have been helped by AAINA where professional treatment has failed. The “revolving door” effect in many substance abuse treatment programs is testimony to the fact that relapse is a common occurrence, even after intensive professional treatment. Some clients keep coming back until their treatment resources are expended (e.g., insurance will no longer pay for treatment or the treatment facility will no longer accept them - some programs have a limit on how many times a person can return). When treatment resources are expended, or it becomes clear that professional treatment is not enough, community resources such as AAINA become more realistic alternatives. AA provides its members with an indigenous support system, and a free therapy which is available anytime. The program goal of AA is clear and focused: to help those who suffer from alcoholism attain sobriety. Following the twelve steps of AA has helped many achieve this goal (Emrick, 1987). Although many alcoholics who attend AA do get better, more empirical 3 research is needed to determine the mechanisms of change through AAINA, and the types of people most likely to change. To date, there have been very few empirical investigations of the efficacy of AA. However, the burden of experimental proof should not be shouldered by AA alone; professional treatment programs have likewise failed to demonstrate their efficacy through empirical research. In his 1990 review of the research on substance abuse treatment programs, Peele made a sweeping claim that no treatment program currently utilized in the US. had sufficiently demonstrated its effectiveness via experimental research. . Furthermore, he stated that current treatment methods (including Alcoholics Anonymous, psychoeducation, confrontation, disulfiram, group therapy, and individual counseling) were based on accepted historical traditions and folk beliefs rather than proven effectiveness. Other less critical reviewers have suggested that the provision of appropriate (i.e., to the client’s needs) treatment does substantially improve outcomes over no treatment at all, but that no particular treatment approach has been found to be effective for all persons with substance abuse problems (Institute of Medicine, 1990). In terms of mental health practice, the increasing use of AAINA as a treatment agent exacerbates the need for research on its efficacy. Although AA has been heralded as the most effective treatment currently available, the dearth of scientific evidence of its efficacy poses a major concern for practitioners (cf. Bebbington, 1976; Glaser & Ogbome, 1982 for comprehensive review and discussion). AAINA may be a valuable resource for some people with substance 4 abuse problems, but others are unable to utilize or benefit from this resource. A number of investigations have been conducted in attempts to identify characteristics associated with AA involvement and outcome. Unfortunately, these studies have been plagued by inadequate research questions and methodological problems (see literature review for more detailed discussion), such that the efficacy of AA for different types of alcoholics remains an elusive subject. Some authors have argued that persons with the characteristics of the respondents in this study - dually diagnosed African American males - are not likely to go to AAINA, and if they do, they receive little benefit from their involvement (cf. Bartalos, 1992; King, 1983; Swift & Beverly, 1985). However, the issue of AAINA involvement may be complicated by other factors, in addition to person characteristics (cf. Humphreys & Woods, 1993 for discussion of contextual factors such as “racial fit”). Studies on benefit from substance abuse treatment in dually diagnosed samples are inconsistent in their findings, but generally indicate that the dually diagnosed are a difficult to treat population (cf. Mueser, Bellack, & Blanchard for discussion). Clinical literature indicates that persons with severe mental illness are often perceived as difficult, non—compliant, and resistant to both psychiatric and substance abuse treatment (Hellerstein & Meehan, 1987). The challenges posed by clients with dual diagnosis sometimes results in their exclusion from both systems of care. Over the past decade, the number of studies on dual diagnosis has 5 increased dramatically (Minkoff, 1989). However, the impact of dual diagnosis on treatment engagement and outcome is not well understood. Some authors (e.g., Bartalos, 1992) have postulated that dual diagnosis status would pose a particular challenge to AA involvement. One issue that has been noted is that ANNA members often actively promote an anti-drug policy, discouraging the use of psychotropic medication. This policy creates a conflict for many dually diagnosed clients. AA members are encouraged to strive for a drug-free state, use the group, and follow the 12-steps as the primary means of dealing with personal problems, a formidable challenge for the person with a serious mental illness. While the official organization literature indicates that group members with psychiatric problems should ngt be discouraged from psychotropic drug use (Alcoholics Anonymous, 1975), the reaction of other AA members towards those who use psychotropic medications varies and remains a very controversial topic (cf. Buxton, Smith, & Seymour, 1987; Zweben, 1987 for more comprehensive discussion of this issue). These, and other problems make it particularly challenging for persons with dual diagnosis to become fully engaged with AAINA and to receive its potential benefits. Research on patient-treatment fit with AAINA, and data on outcome by person characteristics would greatly enhance our ability to determine “which kinds of individuals, under what circumstances, are likely to respond to AA, by achieving what kinds of goals?’ (Institute of Medicine, 1990, p. 143). As more comprehensive data is amassed to answer these questions, clinicians and 6 counselors will be better able to make appropriate referrals to these organizations, thereby improving the chances of successful outcomes. In terms of research strategies, randomized controlled clinical trials have been suggested as the most optimal approach to assessing patient-treatment fit and treatment outcomes (Institute of Medicine, 1990). Other methods include correlational studies using predictor strategies and/or causal modelling (Miller & Hester, 1986). These methods have been used to deal with issues of efficacy in the field of psychotherapy (cf. Kazdin & Wilson, 1978), and are similarly appropriate to research on substance abuse treatment outcome. The current study is an attempt to address the need for empirical data on AAINA efficacy using correlational methods and causal modelling. Purpose of this Study (1) The first purpose of this study is to provide descriptive information on levels of AAINA participation in a dually diagnosed sample, and to determine if in fact persons with dual diagnosis are less likely to participate in AAINA. This goal will be accomplished by comparing rates and patterns of involvement found in this sample with that reported in the literature on singly diagnosed substance abusing clients. (2) A second goal is to identify factors related to AAINA participation in a dually diagnosed population. It is suggested that chronic and/or severe psychiatric symptomatology will act as a deterrent to sustained participation in self-help. Persons with high levels of psychiatric distress are expected to become only 7 marginally involved with AAINA, and to drop out quicker than their psychologically healthier counterparts. (3) Third, this study seeks to determine whether or not patterns of involvement with AAINA are related to alcohol/drug use and other indices of community functioning. The literature reviewed in the following section leads the researcher to predict that those who do not become consistently involved with AAINA soon after professional treatment will to not receive its benefits. (4) Finally, this study will incorporate program theory, and compare the mechanisms of change suggested by two different treatment models: an integrated treatment model and an AAINA model. The goal is to determine which model is more likely to be operating in this sample of persons with dual diagnosis. REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE According to Chen (1990), in order to understand why a program succeeds or fails, the researcher must have an appropriate understanding of the underiying program theory of the treatment being studied. Understanding program theory offers numerous benefits including: allowing the program theory to be tested in a way that reveals whether failure results from implementation failure or failure of the program theory itself; and to test whether the program is being implemented under conditions in which it is appropriate. While most prior research on AA has not incorporated program theory into its evaluation, an attempt will be made to do so in the present evaluation. The program theory of AA is clearly stated in primary literature produced by members of the fellowship, and in secondary sources. The main goal of AA is to help those who suffer from alcoholism attain sobriety. (It should be noted that while most evaluations of AA have focused on abstinence as the primary outcome variable, AA makes a clear distinction between “dryness” and sobriety or serenity, Kurtz, 1993). Those who attain sobriety are able to admit that they are powerless in relation to alcohol, they accept help from a higher power and from other members of the fellowship, they address constitutional and psychological 9 underpinnings of their desire to drink (e.g., self-centeredness, arrogance, etc.) and restore their sanity, they make amends to others that they have harmed in the process of following an alcoholic lifestyle, they make specific changes in their physical and social environments, they stop drinking, and they carry the AA message to others who are suffering from alcoholism. The suggested method of attaining these sobriety-related goals is by attending AA meetings and following the twelve steps. According to AA literature, those who thoroughly follow the AA program w_ill succeed (Alcoholics Anonymous, 1976). Those who do not succeed have not fully implemented the AA program. In terms of the group structure, AA is far from a monolithic organization. There is great diversity in types of AA groups, types of persons who participate in AA, and in members’ perceptions of what AA does (Beckman, 1980). There is no official “leader“ of AA, nor is there any significant input from professionals in terms of how groups should be run or who should participate. The twelve traditions of AA specify that the organization is to remain decentralized, so as to maintain its autonomy from outside forces and allow individual AA groups to remain autonomous in their structure and function, progressing toward the goal of sobriety in a way that is most appropriate for them. AA is open to anyone who is interested in joining; “the only requirement for membership is a desire to stop drinking" (Alcoholics Anonymous, 1976). While the explicit goal of AA is to help members attain sobriety, the implicit goals of the organization address other domains of functioning as well. DiClemente 10 (1992) identified five domains of functioning which are targeted by AA: (1) symptomatic/situational problems — including changes in alcohol consumption and life consequences; (2) maladaptive cognitions - for example, pride, negative thinking; (3) interpersonal conflicts - by making amends to others; (4) family/systems conflicts - value of common welfare above individual’s; and (5) intrapersonal conflicts - addressing character defects that coincide with alcoholism. A number of studies have been published on AA (relatively few on NA); however, a vast majority are reviews and anecdotal reports as opposed to empirical evaluations. This literature review includes both empirical studies, clinical reports, and other reviews, but will emphasize empirical literature on factors related to AAINA participation, factors related to AAINA outcome, social support, and the efficacy of group therapy for persons with severe mental illness. There have been few clinical reports and hardly any empirical studies on NA. Thus, most of the literature cited in this review refers to Alcoholics Anonymous. While the specific content of AA and NA meetings may differ, AA is the historical predecessor of NA and the groups still share many aspects in common. For instance, the groups adhere to the same basic principles of recovery as written in the Big Book (Alcoholics Anonymous, 1976). In fact, it is known that many AA members also have other drug problems and that persons whose primary problem is with illicit dmg use often attend both groups (Emener & Dickman, 1992). Until there is sufficient empirical evidence to suggest that these groups should be considered separately, it must be assumed that they may be simultaneously considered under 11 one broad rubric which is self-help for addiction, based on the AA model. Description of the Literalrre Search The literature reviewed in this section was identified through: computerized searches on Psyclnfo (1976-1995) and MAGIC (Michigan State University computerized library catalog). Key words used in searching these data bases were: Alcoholics Anonymous, Narcotics Anonymous, substance abuse, drug abuse, alcohol abuse, schizophrenia, mental illness, dual diagnosis, treatment, and group therapy. These key words were used in various combinations and yielded more than 200 articles and 20 books on the selected topics. References obtained from the reference sections of the selected primary sources were also retrieved and used in this review. Factors Relatgl to AA/N_A Outcome Most studies on AAINA involvement and outcome have examined personal characteristics of participants and related those to outcome. Upon review of this literature, one finds a mass of chaos and confusion regarding personal correlates of treatment success. Gibbs and Flanagan (1977) conducted a meta-analytic review of the prognostic indicators of treatment outcome and found that across 45 studies, 208 different predictors had been examined. The studies reviewed had explored all kinds of variables from the usual (e.g., sociodemographic characteristics and social stability, prior treatment history, baseline functioning level, prior abstinence/sobriety, length of drinking career, MMPI and other personality measures, etc.) to the unusual (e.g., visual-motor perception, digit 12 span, degree of fantasy ideation, frequency of contact with mother, and number of sexual partners before marriage). Across the 45 studies, the authors were unable to identify any stable predictors using rather modest criteria (i.e., the predictor being tested in six or more studies, and found to predict success in at least half of these studies). While there were a few predictors that met these criteria (e.g., good employment status, marital stability, fewer arrests, history of AA attendance, and higher social class), for most other predictors, there were equal numbers of studies that found contradictory results or nonsignificant results for the same predictors. The studies that examined outcome from AA and other types of group treatment found socioeconomic status, age, marital status, prior involvement with AA, history of abstinence, and psychiatric status to be related to treatment success. While these variables were significant in some studies, in most cases, there were other studies that found nonsignificant or contradictory results with these same predictors. In commenting on the inconsistency of findings across studies, Gibbs and Flanagan (1977) concluded that the task of identifying stable (and universal?) predictors of alcoholism treatment outcome was almost . impossible. Some of the difficulties with such an endeavor are: each study involves different treatment agents (i.e., therapists with different orientations, personalities, and skill levels), administering different types of treatment, and using different outcome criteria over different follow-up time periods, with different types of clients. Fortunately, in the decades since Gibbs and Flanagan’s review, the range of 13 variables studied seems to have decreased somewhat and researchers are beginning to examine some of the same predictors across studies, thereby corroborating prior research findings. Also, researchers are tending to focus on one type of treatment and are defining the characteristics of the treatment population more consistently. However, in a more recent review of the literature on AA, Emrick (1987) came to conclusions similar to those of Gibbs and Flanagan. The findings in the more recent literature were inconsistent and contradictory. One suggestion from these comprehensive reviews is that simplistic questions such as, “does AA work?” are inappropriate. The researcher must consider contextual factors of the person’s environment, their personal characteristics and pre-existing functioning levels, and the context in which treatment in offered (e.g., is the treatment being implemented correctly, at what dosage, and are there any outside influences such as other concurrent treatment which might blur the effects of AAINA involvement). Literature on psychotherapy outcome has shown that multiple criteria are necessary to evaluate treatment outcome and the relative value of different types of treatment (cf. Kazdin & Wilson, 1978). Some of the most important factors in clinical research are: the specificity of change (i.e., did the therapy ameliorate the problem for which therapy was sought), the clinical significance or importance of the change observed, the breadth of change (i.e., has the client experienced improvement in other aspects of his/her everyday functioning), and the durability of any change associated with the treatment. 14 Kazdin and Wilson (1978) have criticized prior research for emphasizing normative group changes following treatment. They argued that group differences in mean performance on an outcome criterion is an extremely limited method for evaluating the effects of treatment. Empirical studies need to explicate factors related to positive outcome in terms of the proportion and types of treated individuals who change, as well as assessing the quality and duration of that change. These points are particularly relevant to persons with dual diagnoses. Dually diagnosed persons often present with a plethora of problems in various life functioning domains. Thus, in order for treatment to be effective for persons with dual diagnoses, it must improve their functioning in a number of affected life domains. Prior studies of AA have tended to focus on one specific outcome: abstinence, and have not fully examined other possible outcomes related to AA attendance; nor have they examined the outcomes of AA participation for persons with multiple disorders. The remainder of this section will review AAINA outcome studies only, with a particular emphasis on AA as aftercare to inpatient substance abuse treatment. Individual Characteristics and AA Attendance Persons who attend AA have been found to be different in many respects from those who do not attend. Some studies have identified differences in demographics, personality characteristics, problem severity, and treatment history for attenders vs. nonattenders (Polich, Armor, & Braiker, 1980). For instance, some studies have found that AA attenders are more likely to be older males who 15 have higher socioeconomic status, self-identify as alcoholic, are affiliated with a church or religious group, are psychologically healthier, have adequate social networks which they utilize, and have had prior substance abuse treatment (Elal- Lawrence, Slade, & Dewey, 1986; O’Leary et al., 1980; Polich, Armor, 8. Braiker, 1980; Ribisl, 1995; Trice & Roman, 1970) . While factors related to AA attendance have been examined in a number of studies, the findings are inconsistent and more recent data generally refutes the prior findings of significant differences (Emrick, 1989). For instance, Thurstin, Alfano, and Nerviano (1987) failed to find any differences in demographics or psychological characteristics of attenders vs. nonattenders. Emrick, in his 1989 literature review, indicated that sociodemographic differences were not consistently observed (except for age - most studies find that AA attenders are likely to be older) and that drinking-related issues (e.g., loss of control, drinking patterns, level of physiological addiction) also do not consistently predict AA involvement. Emrick, Tonigan, Mongtomery, and Little (1993) reviewed all the empirical literature on AA and concluded that “systematic distinctions between AA affiliates and non affiliates can be identified; although the literature is not currently developed enough to provide us with a composite profile of the most likely AA affiliates” (p.53). While motivation to engage in particular types of treatment has not been well studied, motivation to attain sobriety has long been suggested as an integral factor related to treatment engagement and outcome. Clinicians who treat persons with substance abuse problems frequently lament the problem of denial in alcoholics 16 and drug addicts and often use confrontational techniques in an attempt to help clients face the reality of their problems (i.e., “helping the alcoholic to hit bottom,” Miller, 1985). Motivation is a key component of the stages of change model proposed by Prochaska, DiClemente, and colleagues. These authors’ research on addictive behaviors, including alcoholism, suggests that the client’s pretreatment stage of change (i.e., readiness or motivation to change) is one of the most important determinant of treatment success (Prochaska, DiClemente, & Norcross, 1992). AA program literature also highlights the importance of the alcoholic’s level of motivation to stop drinking. The AA Big Book asserts that those who thoroughly follow the AA program will succeed. Those who do not recover are suspected of not giving themselves completely to the program, and are even said to be “constitutionally incapable of being honest with themselves” (Alcoholics Anonymous, 1976). Ditcomes when AA is Aftercare to Professional Treatment AA is commonly used as an adjunct to or follow-up of professional substance abuse treatment. Treatment follow-up studies have probably contributed the most to our fund of knowledge about the efficacy of Alcoholics Anonymous. These studies have revealed a number of important findings. regarding use of AA as treatment aftercare: (1) persons who receive inpatient treatment for substance abuse problems are likely to be referred to AA, and the majority do attend (estimates as high as 80%; Edwards et al., 1971; Knouse & Schneider, 1987; Pettinati, Sugerman, 17 DiDonato, & Maurer, 1982). (2) there is a high drop out rate from AA, even among those who were referred by treatment staff (estimated 25% or less will attend regulariy; Belasco, 1971; Emrick, 1987; Tomsovic, 1970). Data from the General Service Office of Alcoholics Anonymous indicates that fewer than one half of all persons who come to an AA meeting will remain for even three months (AA, 1989). (3) The most positive benefits from AA are usually obtained when it follows formal treatment (Emrick, 1987; Knouse 8 Schneider, 1987) or is used as an adjunct to professional treatment (Emrick, Tonigan, Montgomery, 8. Little, 1992). In addition, Knouse and Schneider (1987) found that of patients who attended AA after inpatient treatment, those who joined AA soon after hospital discharge (i.e., within the first three months) were most likely to maintain sobriety. AA involvement did not seem to make a difference if initiated after three months in the community. While many studies have found that persons who attend AA have better outcomes (especially abstinence rates) than those who do not (cf. EIaI-Lawrence, Slade, & Dewey, 1986; Gregson & Taylor, 1977; Habennan, 1966; Hoffman, Harrison, & Belille, 1983; McBride, 1991), some controlled studies have failed to find any significant differences based on AA attendance (cf. Ditman, Crawford, Forgy, 8 Maskowitz, 1967; Edwards et al., 1977; Walsh et al., 1991). The cited rates of success vary across studies, depending on a number of factors including client characteristics, patterns of AA involvement, access to other supportsystems, and length of follow-up. Studies that follow-up clients for six months or more post- 18 hospitalization usually find that approximately 70% of respondents resumed drinking (Bateman 8 Petersen, 1971). The factors found to be related to continued abstinence include: age, employment status, length of prior abstinence, and prior history of AA attendance. Factors related to relapse include: negative physical or emotional states, social pressure, unemployment, and history of AA attendance (ElaI-Lawrence, Slade, 8 Dewey, 1986; Ogbome 8 Bomet, 1982; Rather 8 Shennan,1989) While most substance abuse treatment outcome studies (especially those studies involving AA) have focused exclusively on drinking outcomes, some authors have stressed the need to examine psychological, social, and behavioral adjustment as well (e.g., Belasco, 1971; Bromet, Moos, Bliss, 8 Wuthmann, 1977). Outcomes in these domains are generally less consistent than drinking outcomes, nevertheless, they warrant mention. In a study of 429 treated alcoholics, Bromet, Moos, Bliss, and Wuthmann (1977) found that those who attended AA after discharge were significantly less likely to be rehospitalized. Emrick, Tonigan, Montgomery, and Little’s (1992) meta-analysis of AA studies found positive relationships between AA involvement and employment (I; = .12) and social/family adjustment (5 = .13). More robust and reliable correlations were found between AA involvement and improvement in psychological adjustment (1 = .25). Some researchers have suggested that the relationship between AA attendance and psychosocial functioning is moderated by attainment of abstinence. That is, AA involvement leads to abstinence which lead to better functioning in 19 other life domains (see Laundergan, 1992 for review). Overall, these findings suggest that the breadth of outcomes attained from AA attendance extend well beyond simple indices of drinking or abstinence, and warrants further investigation. Levels of Involvement and Outcome Patterns of AA attendance have been found to predict drinking outcomes fairly consistently. Persons who attend AA regularly report higher rates of abstinence, fewer days drinking, and fewer episodes of intoxication than those who do not attend regularly (Emrick, 1987; Knouse 8 Schneider, 1987; McBride, 1991; Thurstin, Alfano, 8 Nerviano, 1987; Trice 8 Roman, 1970). McBride (1991) found that the length of continuous AA attendance was highly correlated with months of abstinence (r = .71) and accounted for 50% of the variance in outcome. Conversely, infrequent or irregular attendance at AA post-treatment has been associated with poor prognosis and outcome (McLatchie 8 Lomp, 1988). Emrick, Tonigan, Montgomery, and Little’s (1992) meta-analysis of quantitative studies of AA (g=107) found that frequency of AA attendance and drinking outcomes were correlated r = .19; however, the authors noted that this correlation might be an underestimation. (Due to inconsistency across studies, the correlation contained a high degree of error variance.) There were too few data on AA participation and outcome in other life domains to enter into the meta-analysis. In an earlier review paper, Emrick (1987) suggested that of those who become long-term (not defined), active members of AA, 40-50% will enjoy years of sobriety, and 60-68% will improve somewhat. Similarly, Thurstin, Alfano, and 20 Nerviano (1987) found that persons who attended AA consistently throughout an 18-month follow-up period had the best outcomes of any group. Hoffman, Harrison, and Belille (1983) found that 73% of respondents who attended AA weekly during a 6-month post-hospital follow-up remained chemical-free, while only 33% of nonattenders abstained. One can generally conclude from the literature on AA that people who “work the program,” and attend meetings consistently over a longer period of time are more likely to have better outcomes, at least in terms of their drinking (Emrick, Tonigan, Montgomery, 8 Little, 1992). flmmaiv of Literature on AA Involvement and Outcome The literature on AA reveals considerable evidence that AA attendance helps at least some alcoholics (e.g., those who become regular and active members) to attain abstinence. However, this literature has generally neglected outcomes in other life domains. Although abstinence has been related to better social, physical, and psychological outcomes in some reports (of. Alford, 1980; and Akerlind, Hamquist, Elton, 8 Bjurulf, 1990 for review), neither abstinence nor level of alcohol consumption alone are sufficient predictors of functioning in other life domains. Researchers need to examine other areas of functioning directly instead of using alcohol involvement as a proxy for global functioning. Additionally, there is a need for empirical studies with dually diagnosed persons. Many persons who seek treatment have dual problems, and it is likely that their response to treatment will be quite different from persons with substance abuse problems only. The literature on persons with dual diagnoses of mental 21 illness and substance abuse suggests that they experience problems across a number of life domains, and differ in many respects from other persons with substance abuse problems. Dually diagnosed persons are more likely to be homeless and/or vagrant and to have been jailed (Ridgely, Goldman, 8 Talbott, 1986). Several authors have noted the high rates of criminal involvement, as well as violent acting out and self-injurious behavior among persons with dual diagnosis in comparison to those with singly diagnosed mental illness or substance abuse problems (Kay, Kalathara, 8 Meinzer, 1989; Minkoff 8 Drake, 1991; Mueser, Bellack, 8 Blanchard, 1992; O’Farrell, Connors, 8 Upper, 1983). In terms of mental health treatment, substance abuse has long been recognized as a complicating factor in the treatment of persons with severe mental illness (cf. Mueser, Bellack, 8 Blanchard, 1992; Ridgely, Goldman, 8 Talbott, 1986). Persons with dual problems of substance abuse and mental illness are often more difficult to diagnose and also pose complications to regular treatment of mental illness (e.g., noncompliance with medication regimens). Substance abuse in persons with diagnosed schizophrenia has been related to more severe course of illness, more severe symptomatology, more pervasive deterioration in functioning, and increased likelihood of psychiatric relapse (Mueser, Bellack, 8 Blanchard, 1992; Ridgely, Goldman, 8 Talbott, 1986) Osher and Kofoed (1989) indicated that patients with dual diagnoses were also more difficult to engage in substance abuse treatment, particularly 22 abstinence-oriented treatment. The authors suggested that dually diagnosed patients were less able to transcend denial, a core requirement of many substance abuse treatment programs, because of their “impaired ability to process information due to thought disorders, depressive cognitions, or organic brain syndromes“ (Osher 8 Kofoed, 1989, p. 1028). These authors surmised that prolonged abstinence, the primary goal of AA, would be particularly difficult for persons with dual diagnosis to attain. Treating Persons with Psychiatric Disomgs McLellan and his associates have conducted numerous studies which indicate that high psychiatric severity (assessed by number, intensity, and duration of symptoms) is negatively related to substance abuse treatment outcomes. McLellan et al. (1983) found that in a VA sample of male alcoholics (n=460) and drug addicts (n=282), scores on a psychiatric severity index were strongly and consistently related to treatment outcomes (as measured by seven composite scores from the Addiction Severity Index). This study revealed that patients with low psychiatric severity experienced better treatment outcomes than those with high or mid-range psychiatric severity, no matter what type of treatment they received. In addition, for the high psychiatric severity group, having a greater number of prior treatments was also associated with poorer outcomes. Those with high psychiatric severity showed virtually no improvement in any of the treatment conditions. Overall, psychiatric severity accounted for 48-54% of the variance in treatment outcomes for the high psychiatric symptom group. 23 Using prior psychotherapy as a proxy for psychological health, Haberrnan (1966) found that history of involvement in psychotherapy was negatively related to substance abuse treatment outcome (i.e., abstinence). In Gibbs and Flanagan’s 1977 meta-analytic review, psychiatric history and symptomatology were also found to be negative predictors of treatment outcome in a number studies. Using proxies for psychological well-being such as: having a primary psychosis diagnosis, history of seeking psychiatric treatment, rating oneself as “mentally ill,” scores on personality measures such as the MMPI, and history of suicide attempts, several studies found that better psychological health was related to better outcomes from AA and other group therapies for alcohol abuse. In terms of treatment engagement, some studies have found experience of psychiatric symptoms to be negatively related to engagement in substance abuse treatment (Liss, 1979; Panepinto, Higgins, Keane-Dawes, 8 Smith, 1970). Persons with schizophrenia were more likely become withdrawn or drop out of treatments which stressed collective responsibility or high levels of interpersonal engagement with staff and other clients. If the treatment involved only minimal engagement (e.g., ten-minute sessions with an internist that focused on symptom update, and involved only minimal exploration of thoughts and feelings, clients with schizophrenia were more likely to continue coming (Panepinto et al., 1970). Persons with less severe mental illness were more likely to drop out of the. latter type of treatment. These findings suggests some interesting relationships between client characteristics, type of treatment, and likelihood of treatment success. 24 Eychiatric Svmptomatologymd Interpersonal Functioning Previous studies have demonstrated that persons with chronic mental illness or dual diagnoses are likely to suffer deficits in many areas of life functioning, including family/social relationships (Westerrneyer 8 Neider, 1988). Persons who report experiencing severe symptomatology are more likely to rate their family and social relationships as poor/nonsupportive, and/or conflictual. Participants in the current study were found to have inadequate social networks which were unusually small and unsupportive (Ribisl, 1995). They reported that their relationships were not stable, nor did they serve the important functions of social support or mental health promotion. These types of relationships may actually contribute to experienced distress instead of acting as a buffer against it. Prior research has indicated that experience of interpersonal conflict, especially in the family context, is often a precursor to relapse and is generally related to poorer long-term outcomes for persons with serious mental illness (Swindle, Cronkite, 8 Moos, 1989). While the amount of social support received is affected by contextual factors and the availability of supportive others, it is also affected by individual skills in accessing and maintaining supportive relationships (Heller 8 Swindle, 1983) . Naturalistic studies suggest that persons with better mental health (e.g., social competence and intrapersonal resources) are more likely to establish and maintain supportive social networks (Heller, 1979). Persons with good mental health are more likely to be sociable, assertive, comfortable with intimacy, free from 25 debilitating social anxiety, have good conversational skills, are able to talk about their behavior and feelings, have adequate social problem-solving skills, and are able to take the perspectives of other people (Heller 8 Swindle, 1983). All of these qualities are essential to the initiation and maintenance of social relationships, and social support. Persons with poor mental health are often less socially competent, less adept at interpreting social cues or altering their interpersonal behaviors, and more prone to inaccurate appraisals of social situations. In addition to person characteristics, ecological and community factors also affect the availability and quality of supportive relationships (cf. Korte, 1978 for review). For instance, in highly urbanized areas, isolation and anonymity are common experiences. In an environment where suspicion and hostility are more common than helpfulness and friendliness towards others, developing support networks outside of the family can become a formidable task. Neighborhood attractiveness, density, and racial composition are also important factors that influence the quality of social relationships and social support. It has been suggested that persons with serious and/or chronic mental illness are less efficient at accessing and maintaining social relationships that provide appropriate levels of support to aid in coping. While persons with mental illness may have access to as much social support as others, they are less likely to give support to others or maintain reciprocal social relationships (Heller 8 Swindle, 1983). In addition, respondents in the current study were drawn from an urban metropolitan area in the Midwest, where environmental barriers to social support 26 were likely to exist. Therefore, it was predicted that the participants in this study would be less likely to have successful experiences in the context of a mutual self- help group like AA or NA, where provision and receipt of social support are necessary. Emmetric Svmptomatologya_nd Involvement in GrogLTherapy Clinical literature generally suggests that persons with serious mental illness are often not appropriate for group work (cf. Kanas, 1982; Yalom, 1985). This finding is quite relevant to investigating the appropriateness of AA for a group of persons with problems of mental illness as well and substance abuse. Stnrcturally and philosophically, there are some important differences between professional psychotherapy and self-help or mutual aid groups (e.g., no formal group “leader” or therapist, self-selection to group). Self-help groups have traditionally been defined as being composed of members who share a common problem, create a network of support for one another through regular social and emotional interactions, are self-goveming, and involve egalitarian relationships based on principles such as “helper therapy” and identification of members as “prosumers” (i.e., producer as well as consumers of services and aid; Hedrick, lsenberg, 8 Martini, 1992; Leiberrnan 8 Borman, 1979). Another major difference is that self-help programs are free and open to all who wish to attend. Nevertheless, there are many elements which AA shares in common with other therapy groups (Knight, Wollert, Levy, Frame, 8 Padgett, 1980; Yalom, 1985). Irvin Yalom’s work has been most instrumental in identifying the curative 27 elements involved in group therapy process - that is, the mechanisms of change that can be found in anytherapy group. Of the eleven factors identified by Yalom, at least nine can be found in AA self-help groups: instillation of hope, universality, imparting of information, altruism, development of socializing techniques, imitative behavior, interpersonal learning, group cohesiveness, and catharsis. (The remaining two factors are somewhat tangential to the emphasis of AA - corrective recapitulation of primary family group and existential experiences.) While there is no group leader present to ensure that these elements are in effect, there is a standard protocol to most AA groups which includes these factors. For instance, the use of testimonials of the effectiveness of AA serves to instill hope in other members, especially new or struggling members. The common use of the introduction “hi, my name is _ and I’m an alcoholic” serves to reinforce a sense of commonality, group cohesion, and validation of the experiences of other members. Imparting of advice and altruistic behaviors (e.g., sponsorship, 12- stepping - saving other alcoholics by telling them about AA) are also common experiences in AA. One of the most important, if not the critical curative ingredient of AA is involvement in mutual social support and fellowship with other members (Knight, Wollert, Levy, Frame, 8 Padgett, 1980; Maxwell, 1984). As noted previously, AA also serves as a primary support group for many members, fulfilling many of their material, social, and psychological needs (Emrick, 1989). Those who have studied AA groups, particularly via participatory observation, find that AA members derive 28 benefit primarily by involving themselves in fellowship with other members (e.g., through sponsorship, 12-stepping, etc.). Maxwell (1984), a psychologist who conducted studies on AA process, indicated that recovery through AA “means becoming immersed in the AA social environment. It means participating in open, honest sharing and interaction with fellow members.” Furthermore, “the degree and quality of involvement with other members will be a major factor in how much will be gained from AA” (p.155). 1 Clinical literature suggests that group work is not the treatment of choice for persons with serious and/or chronic mental illness, especially those with schizophrenia (cf. Otteson, 1979 for review). Clinicians have noted that “among the general characteristics of the chronically mentally ill is their difficulty in becoming meaningfully engaged in a therapeutic endeavor” (Stone, 1991, p.13). Often persons with severe mental illness are not able to withstand the anxieties related to self-disclosure or confrontation, which are common elements of group experience. Several theories have been proposed to explain this resistance to group engagement. For example, it has been proposed that persons with mental illness are more concerned about survival than self-realization; that the level of developmental arrest and intrapersonal conflict inhibits the establishment of trusting relationships and also makes it difficult for persons with mental illness to regulate personal boundaries without feeling anxious; and finally, that psychiatric patients experience a realistic caution against revealing themselves, since prior experience has taught them that this is dangerous and may lead to punishment (Chacko, 29 Adams, 8 Gomez, 1985; Stone, 1991). Another hypothesis is that use of psychotropic medications subdues both abnormal and normal behaviors, including social responsiveness. In terms of group process, the work of Yalom and his colleagues indicates that when people with psychosis and other severe mental illnesses are placed in groups where most members are not suffering from mental illness, the peculiarity of the person with mental illness is often obvious to other group members. Oddities in dress, mannerisms, and verbalizations (e.g., irrelevant or bizarre verbal content) cause them to stick out like sore thumbs. Thus, they often find it difficult to fit in the group and are quickly ostracized as deviants. (In terms of AA, the person .with mental illness might also be taking psychotropic medication, which, if discovered would also make them different from the other group members and also inhibit their adherence to the group norm of abstinence from all chemicals.) Additionally, persons with severe mental illness, especially those on psychotropic medications, often appear to be “sealed over" (Yalom, 1985, p. 237). Their affect may be flat, inappropriate or bizarre and they may be generally withdrawn from social interactions. Clinicians who work specifically with psychotic clients indicate that they often have deficits in even the most mundane social and conversational skills (Wilson, Diamond, 8 Factor, 1990). The inability of many persons with severe mental illness to involve themselves in an interactional group without threatening the group norms or becoming overly anxious themselves, seriously interferes with their ability to derive benefit from a group therapy experience. According to Yalom, 30 the psychotic person tends to interfere with the normal dynamic process of the group and consequently, “eventually the group will extrude the deviant” (1985, p. 238). Thus, one would expect these persons to be forced out by the group or to opt out of the group themselves. Specific to self-help group involvement, Bartalos (1992) suggests that “in order to be an effective member of a self-help group, one has to have adequate perception and judgement, ability to interact meaningfully with others and a reasonable chance to benefit from such interactions" (p. 72). Given this proscription for successful participation, persons with severe mental illness would be among those left out (by self-selection or a group screening process) due to inability to participate fully. Although much clinical evidence suggests that group therapy with seriously mentally ill and/or dually diagnosed persons is extremely difficult, some clinicians and researchers have been successful in developing models of group work that are effective with these patients (e.g., Fairweather et al., 1969; Hellerstein 8 Meehan, 1987; Otteson, 1979; Wilson, Diamond, 8 Factor, 1990; Yalom, 1985). These successful efforts generally have been developed and utilized with homogeneous groups (e.g., hospitalized psychiatric patients) and involve special techniques designed to address the particular needs of persons with serious mental illness. The Fairweather Lodge studies were perhaps the first attempt by researchers to determine whether a group of chronically mentally ill patients could function in a community-based group. Fairweather and his colleagues found that a 31 group of chronically mentally ill patients (more than half also had substance abuse problems), when given the chance, were able to live semi-autonomously and ultimately take complete responsibility for their community, and their livelihood. In terms of outcome, across a 40-month hospital follow-up period, the Lodge members were more successful in maintaining employment and remaining in the community (i.e., spending more days in the community than in the hospital) than a comparison group that received traditional aftercare services. Hellerstein and Meehan (1987) found similar results with a group of dually diagnosed patients. Using a group model designed especially to address dual diagnosis issues, these authors reported success in reducing the number of days participants spent in the hospfial Although the Lodge project was successful overall, there were differential outcomes based on chronicity and severity of illness. Persons with more chronic mental illness (i.e., having been in the hospital longer) spent fewer days in the community, one of the most important measures of successful outcome, and were less likely to be employed than those with more acute illness. Behaviorally, the less chronic and nonpsychotic persons were more adept at handling the social demands of Lodge living and work environments. They were more likely to chosen by their peer to be leaders in the Lodge community (45% of nonpsychotic Lodge members were selected as leaders vs. only 15% of psychotic members). Given these differential outcomes, the authors concluded that "chronicity places a damper on friendship relations, verbal contact, activity level, and freedom from emotional 32 disturbance," all of which are requirements of communal living or work settings (Fairweather et al., 1969). Other authors have identified successful models for group work with hospitalized psychiatric patients (cf. Wilson, Diamond, 8 Factor, 1990), in which the therapists use special techniques to help clients manage anxiety and learn appropriate social skills via step-by-step skills training. In terms of AA involvement, Minkoff (1989) found that with special preparation in advance, dually diagnosed patients were able to participate successfully and benefit from AAINA. Staff at this specialized program prepared clients for AAINA attendance by: providing them with individualized education on appropriate behavior in the group, having them participate in groups with other dually diagnosed persons, linking them up with another person in AAINA, and selecting meetings that would be most suitable for dually diagnosed persons. These methods eased the transition of dually diagnosed persons into the groups, allowing them to feel more comfortable and providing them with behavioral coaching to minimize inappropriate and/or bizarre behaviors which might frighten other group members. Unfortunately, no specific data on patient outcomes were reported in this study. Summary This review suggests that although persons with severe and chronic mental illness are generally not expected to respond well to group therapy, under special circumstances and with advanced training in social skills, people with chronic mental illness or dual diagnosis may be able to benefit from mutual aid groups such 33 as AAINA. More empirical research is needed to determine the extent and conditions under which AAINA can be utilized as a therapeutic resource by persons with dual diagnosis. . Limitations of Prior Studies on AA The majority of reports on the efficacy of AA have been anecdotal and/or inconclusive due to lack of scientific rigor. Miller and Hester (1980) identified three very serious problems with the literature on AA: (1) lack of control groups in experimental studies, (2) reliance on abstinence as the sole criteria of treatment success, and (3) over reliance on self-report measures of both AA attendance and outcomes. Another important issue is that the effectiveness of a particular treatment will be affected by whether this is the only type of treatment received. Concurrent involvement in other types of treatment can obscure results, and is particularly problematic in terms of evaluating multimodal treatment programs in which AAINA is only one component (Emrick, 1987; Thurstin, Alfano,8 Nerviano, 1987). The effects of AA are often confounded with the effects of the other components of the treatment program. One of the major obstacles to empirical investigation of AA is inaccessibility of AA members. This limitation has resulted in many studies using biased samples - either volunteers (self-selection bias) or court referred participants (who might be quite different from other members). Additionally, most studies have been conducted with socially stable, Caucasian males. To date, there have not been any studies of the efficacy of AA for persons who are dually diagnosed. 34 gigala of the Present Stady This study will address some of the weaknesses identified in prior research. One of the major criticisms of prior research is a failure to incorporate program theory into research evaluations. The current study will compare two treatment models. In light of the stated program theory and goals of AA and those of the specialized treatment received by the study participants, an attempt will be made to test a model of integrated treatment (see Figure 1, Model A) for persons with dual diagnoses as opposed to a model which is more consonant with AA program theory (see Figure 2, Model B). This study will examine whether AAINA attendance, following specialized treatment, relates directly to outcomes in other functioning domains (e.g., psychological and family/social functioning, and recidivism), as would be suggested by an integrated treatment model, or whether these outcomes are mediated by abstinence or improvement in drinking/drug use, as would be suggested by an AAINA treatment model. The proposed model for this study (see Figure 1) was adapted from the work of Moos and his colleagues. According to Moos, Finney, and Maude-Griffin (1992), the individual’s personal characteristics and life context interacts with treatment to produce differential outcomes. Specifically, participation in treatment is influenced by the individual’s resources prior to entering the group (noted in the model as individual characteristics/prior experiences), and the contextual factors that concur with treatment involvement (here noted by concurrent engagement in other forms of treatment besides AAINA). Individual characteristics and prior 35 treatment experiences are expected to correlate with involvement in aftercare services, particularly AAINA. These characteristics will influence the level of participation in aftercare treatment, as well as the types of services used. Service use is expected to be related to subsequent functioning in the treated domains (i.e., alcohol/drug use, psychological functioning, etc). The present study will examine the extent to which pre-existing characteristics can be used to predict subsequent service use, and whether service use has any significant impact on outcomes. (The specific measures of each construct are depicted in the lower part of the figure, enumerated in the square boxes). 36 gonad—833:0.“ . 2.3395 70332—9»: .8038.— _a_oom\.£8am nae—no.5 man .9328 .282 v-a'oi'aiv'vn .332 3258.5. 3338.: 2:. . < .362 A 8.83 m 25. a )~ .2 23.8.... :8 833025 .4 «5:.— botaom .m 352.8; mn. 0...0F I F0. .0.- 0w. #0. 0? 0..- 00.- N... 00. 00. 0f- 00.- 00.- F0. mooiuao 00.0.. I 0? 00.- 00.- 0%. of. Sr 00. 00. F0. 00.- 0w. 00.- 90.- 321530.23 I 00.- 00. 0v. N0. 00. ..0.- 0...- N0.- .0. 3.... N. 0... goo/42300.0? I 0.. 0N.- 00.- 0N; 3. N0.- 00. 0? 00.- 5.- 00. 5:03...“ 0h.N—. I _.N.- N0. 0v. 0? 3.- 00.- vor 00.- 0?. 00.- Lo.aw<<0...: I 0v. .0. 00. 00. 00.- 00. 00.- 0... N0. cch<<0POF I F0. 0? v0.- 00.- 3. 00.- 0N. 00. 58.371670 I N? 00. Sr 0?. 00.- 0... 3. c.o>w<<.0 I 00. N0. 00.- ..0. 00.- 00.. .o_co.=.0\xw 505$ I 00. 00.- 0...- N0.- 00. 0503.5 20.... .0 I .0.- v0. 00.- 00.. .0299.me 30.. .0 I 00.- 09.- ..0. 0520.0 26.. d I 0... VN; wcfln. 30.500 .0 I .0; . 5.0.-poi c0223 macaw—0:00 <- 0.9m... 72 Hypotheses l 8 ll: Relationship of Individual Characteristics to ANN_A Attendance It was predicted that individual characteristics, that is, psychiatric symptoms and history of psychiatric hospitalization would be significant predictors of ANNA attendance during the follow-up period. Persons who reported experiencing high levels of psychiatric symptoms and multiple prior psychiatric hospitalizations were expected to be less likely to ever attend ANNA during the 10-month follow-up period, while those with fewer symptoms and more acute psychiatric problems were expected to be most likely to attend ANNA. This hypothesis was tested via logistic regression with the three symptom/chronicity design variables as the independent variables (the low symptom/acute group was used as the constant), treatment condition, prior ANNA attendance, and sobriety plans as the covariates, and ANNA Ever as the outcome variable. The covariates were entered together on step one; the symptom/chronicity groups were entered on step two. Table 8 shows the results of each step of the logistic regression predicting ANNA Ever. Included in each step are the regression coefficients (B), the standard errors, and the partial correlations (R) for each independent variable. The B values may be interpreted as the log odds of the event occurring (i.e., the participant ever attends ANNA after hospitalization), given a one-unit increase in the predictor variable. The three covariates entered on step one were significant predictors of ANNA Ever, classifying 62% of the respondents correctly. Of the covariates entered on this step, prior ANNA attendance was the only significant predictor. The log odds of a participant ever attending ANNA after hospitalization were 73 increased by .97 if they had attended ANNA prior to hospitalization. Treatment condition (i.e., dual diagnosis treatment vs. regular psychiatric treatment) and having plans to maintain sobriety did not significantly predict the odds of attending ANNA. On step two, there was a slight improvement in classification of ANNA attenders vs. nonattenders with the addition of the symptom/chronicity groups. The percent correctly classified increased to 63%. Of the symptom/chronicity groups, membership in the high symptom/chronic group was significantly related to ANNA attendance. The log odds of ANNA attendance decreased by .94 for individuals in this group. While the coefficients for the other groups were not significant,- all were in the same direction, indicating that persons in the low symptom/chronic and high symptom/acute groups were also less likely to attend ANNA. However, the log odds for the constant was positive (though statistically nonsignificant), indicating that persons in the low symptom/acute group were somewhat more likely to attend ANNA than the other groups. 74 Table 8. Logistic Regression of Symptom/Chronicity and Covariates on ANNA Ever (p = 283) . Step1 x2 Improvement = 11.18“ 6822 i x2 Improvement = 7.79“ Classified 61.84% Classified 63.25% B S.E. R B S.E. R BEG—kl Tx Condition .12 .27 .00 .12 .28 .00 Prior ANNA .97“ .32 .14 1.09“ .33 .16 Sobriety Plans -.10 .26 .00 -.04 .26 .00 Constant .35 .47 88811.2. Low leChronic -.58 .37 -.03 High Sx/Acute -.44 .36 .00 High Sx/Chronic -.94* .35 -.12 Constant .72 .51 *p<.05 75 The second hypothesis was that persons in the high symptom/chronic group would be less likely to become regular ANNA attenders, if they ever attended at all. It was predicted that persons in the more severe/chronic groups would be more likely to drop out of ANNA than persons in other symptom/chronicity groups. This hypothesis was tested via ANCOVA using the 4-level symptom/chronicity variable as the independent factor, treatment condition, prior ANNA attendance, and sobriety plans as the covariates, and ANNA Pattern as the dependent variable. Table 9 shows the results of this analysis. As the results for hypothesis l indicated, persons in the high symptom/chronic groups attended ANNA less consistently after hospitalization than members of the other symptom/chronicity groups. Persons in the low symptom/acute group were the most consistent in their ANNA attendance. The overall model was statistically significant and explained 8.0% of the variance in ANNA attendance patterns. Hymtheses III 8 IV: Relationship of ANNA amt Other Service Use to Functioning Hypothesis three indicated that persons who became consistent ANNA attenders soon after hospitalization would show better outcomes than those who never attended ANNA or who attended sporadically. This hypothesis was tested via five separate MANOVA models with ANNA attendance pattern as the independent factor and the five sets of outcome measures (e.g., Time 3 alcohol problems and Time 4 alcohol problems) as the dependent variables. The results of these analyses are reported in Table 10. The MANOVA results indicate only 76 mo. v m. .965. some 8.. names 9m 2pm. 5 82; ”9oz 8. L B u m emanaxm :33 255 .858 B... u m. 8565...... B. .8... u. <22... BE 8. u. 8:680 up mm.mtm>_ oo Eozma S. .8...” u m 8. mm. 9.. 8.. 8:885. <23. “mm .84 .2 u 9 .8 u s .8 u 9 .8 u s o_8.=m> 88:85 2:25 5 Si 2:955 33 28.0.5 :9: 2:25 33 .mmw u s «.2898 a 8:20 aaa€5§§e$ so... E88 8:88.... 4.2298628 m<>ooz< .8 28A no. v m. .0380 comm 6.. ENE 2... can. 5 mo:_m> No.02 77 00. 0N. 3‘. 00. N0. .V 0E... 00. NN. 00. 5‘. 0N. 0 0E... 00:050.. wocmm 050.205.”. 2... mm. mm. a. om. v as: 8. 8. cm. mm. mm. m 95 weaned 8825...... «3 mm. mm. 8. B. e 8&5 8. mm. mm. 8. on. m 9:5 mEoBBd _mo_ 29.0 mn. me. 2. S. S. S. 48.5 o: 2. 3. NF. 3. mos: 953. 8d: 020 E. mm. 8. 8. ea. v as: 8. bad 2. m... 8. em. m 9:: weaned 6:82 New L .8 u a .8 n s .8 u 9 .B. u s 285; 95895.. 95035.. .5865... 80:92 0 __m .m 8392 90:63:00 F .m 63:32. .652 N a. 8822 .48 n s 58$ 88222 <22... 52.. 3885“. 8:285 252% .9 285 mo. v m. .955 comm :2 959: 2m 999 5 mm:_m> ”Boz 78 3. mm. mm. 8. mm. v 9:5 m: 5.. mm. mm. «a. m 9:5 8:85 8%m 9.630%: 9. om. a. mm. a. v 9:5 S. cm. mm. «N. «N. m 9:5 mien. aboflzfilfl. E. 5. mm. mu. 3.. v 9:5 8.5 5. mm. mm. 5.. m 9:5 95.205 $5 205 mn. mad 2. N5. 8. 5. v 9:5 3. .3: 2. 9. mo. 5. m 9:5 wEmEEn. 9:0 39 mm. vm. om. 8. v 35 3. a; mm. a. 9. mm. m 9:5 mEmaQa .0282 "mm. n. a: u s an n 3 8: u s 62 u s 29%.; 2:0 wmo_>._mw mmo_>..mw 2:0 (23 0:02 .mcofimmh—oi .mcofimmBE + «59:33:. Immfimcg 9:850 n 9:...- Ava n S Eaton. 005282 (2)3 5:5 A3 .035. 98958.... ”£33m mmzas‘ 5am .9. aim-r 88 820.02: .882 v... 2 020.02: .0:8.< n... 22.. £0: 05 :00. 50200 0200 05 5 «20.02: 2. >0 0.20.02: E. :0 88.02: 05 E8052 88.0508 50: 80:... . . Po. :05 mmo. $030508 500 masofic. I .mo.v d «a uconcmmm «00.05000 50:: c 2. 8. 8. 8. 3.. a _:0 8.- 8.- 8.- 5.- 5.- .. .:22 38¢: .88: 59v: .885 53: 8:8 53: .855 5.7: .288 u 8.2,: .8 a: .0. I9: .892 5. 5.- 8. 8. 5.- 98.85 :9: :5 5. I 5. 8. I 8.25 :9: :5 8. 5.- 8. 8. I 9:2:05 33 :5 ”I880 52.2. .8. .8. .8. .8. .8. 8.29:2: 2 5. 8.- 8. .3 F. 5. 2:0 802:: 89822: 5. 5. 8.- 8. 8. 892:0 89822: + << 5. 5. 5.- 8. 5. 2:0 5. I880 .890 8. 5. 5. 8. 5.- 9:285 :9: :5 5. I 8. 8. 5.- 825 :9: :5 8. I 5. 8. I 9:285 26.. :5 I825 62.2. .3. 8. 8. .2. 3. 3.- 9.:0 892:: 89822: 3. 5. .3. 8. .8 3.- 892:0. 3:99.22: + S. 8. 8. 8.- 8.- 8.- 2:0 << I880 0890 8855.808: 020.02: 80an: 020.02: 85: 020.02: :20 8.0.02: .0:8_< 8.05:...) E00:0:00:. Im0_I0m:m> 02850 n 02.... $8 a s n :2: .: 3: 8.2:: .o 8.; 5.; .5 .892 3252:. ”9.8:: 98:2 5:: .3 9:8 89 Overall, the hypothesized path models did not prove to be good fits with the data. All of the Chi-squares were large and highly significant, indicating that the null hypothesis of no relationship between the variables could not be rejected. While the overall models were not good fits, there was some consistency with findings from the previous analysis. For instance, persons in the high symptom/chronic group were significantly less likely to attend AAINA regularly (B = -.17). They were also significantly more likely to use professional services only (B = .25), as were persons in the low symptom/chronic group (B = .19). However, in most cases, symptom/chronicity had very negligible impact on functioning via service use, as indicated by the low indirect effects. The previously identified relationships between AAINA service use and functioning were generally supported with the path analysis results. For instance, persons who attended AAINA more often at Time 3 had fewer alcohol problems (B = -.15, see Table 12). Some divergent findings regarding service use and outcomes also emerged from the path analyses. For instance, regarding type of service use (Table 14), the path from AAINA only to Time 3 drug problems was not significant, as would be expected based on previous analysis. However, persons who used professional services only had significantly more drug problems at Time 3 (B = .17) and Time 4 (B = .11). Also, persons who used AAINA in addition to professional services had more psychological problems at Time 3 (B = .14). And those who used professional services only (which were predominantly outpatient mental health services) were more likely to be rehospitalized at Time 3 (B é .14). 90 The path models confirmed the lack of relationship between service use and Time 4 functioning. None of the direct paths from service use to Time 4 functioning reached statistical significance. The only relationships identified with Time 4 functioning were indirect paths, via Time 3 functioning. The high correlations between Time 3 and Time 4 functioning accounted for these relationships. Tables 15, 16, and 17 show the path analysis results for the ANNA Treatment models in which Time 3 alcohol and drug problems were mediators of functioning in the other domains. None of these models were judged to be significant fits with the data, as indicated by the large chi-squares and low values on the fit indices. However, there were more significant paths in the AAINA Treatment models than in the Integrated Treatment models, primarily due to the concurrent relationships between the various indices of Time 3 functioning. For instance, Time 3 alcohol problems were significantly related to Time 3 psychological problems. Time 3 drug problems were significantly related to Time 3 psychological, family/social problems, and psychiatric rehospitalization. However, no significant relationships were identified for Time 4 functioning. In these. path models, as in the Integrated Treatment models, the indirect effects of symptom/chronicity were very small or nonexistent, as were the indirect effects of service use. 91 Table 15. Path Analysis Results: AAINA Treatment Model (B) with Number of Days Attended A/NA at Time 3 (fl = 284) Time 3 Outcome Variables Independent Variables Psych Problems FamlSoc Problems Rehospitalization Direct Effects: T3 Alcohol Problems .15“ .08 .03 T3 Drug Problems .27* 22* .22* Mirect Effects: Via T3 AAINA Days -.04 -.02 -.01 Via Low leChronic --- --- .. Via High Sx/Acute -- ... .. Via High leChronic -- ... .- Time 4 Ogtcome Variables Dfiirect Effects: T3 Alcohol Problems .09 .05 .05 T3 Drug Problems -.01 -.07 -.03 T3 Problems‘1 .48* .31" .36* mgirect Effects: Via T3 ANNA Days -.03 -.01 -.01 Via Low leChronic -- ... .. Via High Sx/Acute -- ... . .. Via High leChronic -- ... .. Model Fit: x2 (18 df), p value = 189.49, p <.001 160.39, p <.001 177.63, p <.001 NNFI = .09 -.14 -.14 CF I = .42 .27 .27 * Path coefficient significant at g <.05. -- Indicated path coefficients less than .01 ' These path coefficients represent the prediction of T4 problems by T3 problems in the same domain (e.g., the path from T3 Alcohol problems to T4 Alcohol problems). 92 Table 16. Path Analysis Results: AAINA Treatment Model (B) with AAINA Attendance Pattern (g = 284) Time 3 Outcome Variables Independent Variables Psych Problems FamlSoc Problems Rehospitalization QLreCLEffects: T3 Alcohol Problems .15* .08 .03 T3 Drug Problems .27“ .22" .22* lrlqirect Effects: Via AAINA -.04 -.03 -.02 Attendance Via Low Sx/Chronic -- -- -- Via High Sx/Acute —- -- -- Via High Sx/Chronic .01 .01 .01 Time 4 Outcome Variables Direct Effects: T3 Alcohol Problems .09 .05 .05 . T3 Drug Problems -.01 -.07 -.03 T3 Problems' .48* .31" .36* Indirect Effects: Via AAINA -.03 -.01 -.01 Attendance Via Low Sx/Chronic -- --- -- Via High Sx/Acute -- -- -- Via High Sx/Chronic .01 -- -- M9131; x2 (18 df), p value = 189.27, p <.001 158.40, p <.001 175.27, p <.001 NNFI = .09 -.14 -.14 CFI = .41 .26 .27 * Path coefficient significant at g <.05. -- lndicates path coefficients less than .01. ' These path coefficients represent the prediction of T4 problems by T3 problems in the same domain (e.g., the path from T3 Alcohol problems to T4 Alcohol problems). 93 Table 17. Path Analysis Results: AAINA Treatment Model (B) with Type of Service Use at Time 3 (Q = 284) Time 3 (Artcome Variables Independent Variables Psych Problems FamlSoc Problems Rehospitalization Direct Effects: T3 Alcohol Problems .16* .08 .03 T3 Drug Problems .27" .22* .22" Indirect Effects: Via AAINA Only .01 .04 -.02 Via AAINA .05 .03 .02 + Prof Services Via Prof Services Only .07 .05 .04 Via Low Sx/Chronic .01 .02 .01 Via High Sx/Acute .01 -- .01 Via High SxIChronic .01 .02 .01 Time 4 Outcome Variables Direct Effects: T3 Alcohol Problems .09 .05 .05 T3 Drug Problems -.01 -.07 -.03 Time 3 Problems' .48* .31" .36* Indirect Effects: Via AAINA Only .02 .01 -.02 Via AAINA .03 - -- + Prof Services - - Via Prof Services Only .04 -- -- Via Low SxIChronic .01 -- -- Via High leAcute -- -- -- Via High Sx/Chronic .01 -- -- Model Fit: x2 (25 df), p value = 276.94, p <.001 244.89, p <.001 262.69, p <.001 NNFI = -.14 -.34 -.34 CFI = .37 .26 .26 * Path coefficient significant at p <.05. -- Indicates path coefficients less than .01. ' These path coefficients represent the prediction of T4 problems by T3 problems in the same domain (e.g., the path from T3 Alcohol problems to T4 Alcohol problems). 94 Overall, these results indicate that the full models were very poor fits with the data. Using the Wald test of nonsignificant paths, an attempt was made to reach a parsimonious and well-fitted model with each set of variables. All nonsignificant paths were eliminated from each model until an interpretable path that met the criteria for model fit could be identified. These methods resulted in two models that fit the data. Figure 5 shows the significant model predicting psychological functioning, trimmed of all nonsignificant paths. (The full model with all paths shown can be found in Appendix A.) This model showed acceptable fit with the data - x2 (3 df) = 3.49, 9 =32; NNF I = .97; CFI = .99, indicating that it was a plausible model. Persons who attended AAINA more often had fewer alcohol problems at Time 3, and fewer psychological problems. Those with fewer psychological problems at Time 3 also had fewer psychological problems at Time 4. Symptom/Chronicity was not a significant predictor in this model. Although this model met the criteria for acceptable fit, it only accounted for a small amount of the variance in Time 3 alcohol problems (32 = .01) and Time 3 psychological problems (_R_2 = .03), and a moderate amount of the variance in Time 4 psychological problems (32 = .13). The second model that emerged is shown in Figure 6. Persons in the high symptom/chronic group were more likely to use professional services only, which was related to experiencing more drug problems at Time 3 and more family/social problems (x2 (6 df) = 8.06, 9 =23; NNFI = .94; CFI = .97). Similar to the previous model, this trimmed model only accounted for a small percent of the variance in 95 use of Professional services (32 = .02), Time 3 drug problems (32 = .01), Time 3 family/social (i32 = .03), and Time 4 family/social problems (32 = .05). Interestingly, use of AAINA only did not emerge as a significant predictor in the trimmed model, as in previous analyses. 96 ha. \ 3.038." 133235 e 3:. can. .8030; 9.5 m 3:. «6038.— 193235 n 3:. m «MN. \ so. «803°; 3483‘ m 08C. ma. nausea: 333a... 3.3.3:. 5: 329:3 .m 83E 38:63 .3: m :5 <2)? m 25w m 2923 SEW A 38205 33 97 \ 3038.. Enigma: a 2...... can. nae—noun 30835". n as; 5a. ma. wn. aa. a 2.8395 9.5 m cam... N «80305 _2—oo—< m 08E. w 333.5 30835.“ 3.33: a: .8055 s 2:5 am. / 2.5 fl «cerium idemaouoa fleeiufim gnaw O m 2:03am anew masoaohm ad «08.68 13.825 + <35. m as 2.2 w 98 filmmarv of Research Questions and Findings (1) Are the rates of AAINA participation among persons with dual diagnoses comparable to those without any diagnosed mental illness? Yes. The self-reported rates of AA/NA attendance in this sample were Comparable to those reported in the literature on persons with substance abuse disorders. (2) Are persons with severe and chronic mental illness likely to attend AAINA, and if so, how consistent will their attendance be? Participants with severe and chronic psychiatric symptoms were significantly less likely to ever attend AA/NA after hospital discharge. They were also less likely to attend AAINA consistently throughout the 1 0-month follow-up period. Participants with fewer psychiatric symptoms of acute duration reported the most consistent AA/NA attendance. (3) Are patterns of AAINA attendance related to alcohol/dmg use and other indices of community functioning? . Persons who attended AA/NA consistently over the follow-up period (e. g., at all 3 interview timepoints) reported the best outcomes. They had significantly fewer alcohol problems. No significant findings were observed for AA/NA attendance and other functioning domains, although the direction of results for drug and psychological problems and psychiatric hospitalization were in the same direction. The latter findings were not robust enough to reach statistical significance. 99 (4) Do persons who receive professional services in addition to AAINA show better outcomes than those who attend AAINA only? No. Use of professional services was not related to better outcomes than attendance at AA/NA only. In fact, persons who attended AA/NA only reported the best outcomes in terms of alcohol and drug abuse. Those who did not attend any services reported the most problems of any group. There were no significant difference in other functioning domains based on type of service use. (5) Are the mechanisms of change in this sample more consonant with an integrated treatment model or an AAINA model? Findings from the path analyses suggested that the AA/NA treatment model was more consonant with the data. There were no direct relationships between AAINA attendance or other service use and psychological and family/social problems or psychiatric rehospitalization. However, persons who attended AA/NA and reduced their alcohol use also reported significantly better psychological problems. Similariy, there was a mediated relationship between use of professional services, drug problems, and family/social problems. Chapter 4 Discussion This study sought to answer several questions related to AAINA attendance and outcomes among dually diagnosed individuals. While there has been a general expectation that self-help groups such as AAINA would not be appropriate or helpful for persons with the characteristics of this study's respondents, there was almost no empirical literature to validate this assumption. The current study is a step towards building an empirical data base on the efficacy of self-help groups for persons with dual diagnoses. The first purpose of this study was to provide descriptive information on the levels of AAINA participation in this sample, and to determine if persons with dual diagnoses attended AAINA at different rates from persons with singly diagnosed substance abuse disorders. Previous investigators have indicated that of persons referred to AA following inpatient substance abuse treatment, 60-80% will attend (Edwards et al., 1971; Knouse & Schneider, 1987). In the current study, 62% of the respondents attended AAINA at some time during the 10-month follow-up time period, which is comparable to the previously reported rates of attendance among persons with single diagnoses. Pettinati, Sugerman, DiDonato, and Maurer (1982) 100 101 reported AAINA attendance rates of 74% in a dually diagnosed sample. It is somewhat more difficult to compare gt_e_§ or patterns of attendance across studies due to the wide divergence in definitions of “regular” vs. “irregular' attendance. However, the finding that 24% of the respondents in this study attended AAINA at least once every four months over the 10-month follow-up is comparable to rates reported in the literature (Belasco, 1971; Emrick, 1987; Pettinati, Sugerrnan, DiDonato, & Maurer, 1982; Temsovic, 1970). At 10-months post-hospitalization, 7% of the respondents were attending AAINA two times per week or more. These findings are rather exceptional given the fact that most prior studies have been conducted with socially stable Caucasian males with substance abuse problems only. These data suggest that low-functioning, dually diagnosed, African American males are equally likely to attend AAINA. Relationshi between Individual haracteristics and Service Use Several studies have attempted to identify characteristics that differentiate persons who will attend AAINA from those who will not. This study examined several individual characteristics thought to be related to AAINA attendance: exposure to AAINA treatment models via specialized dual diagnosis treatment, prior AAINA attendance, plans to maintain sobriety, psychiatric symptomatology at hospital entry, and the number of prior psychiatric hospitalizations. Of the covariates used in this study, only prior AAINA attendance was significantly related to AAINA attendance during the follow-up time period. Participation in the specialized dual diagnosis treatment and motivation for sobriety (as measured by 102 stated plans to engage in treatment and other activities to maintain sobriety) were not significant covariates in the models predicting AINA attendance. The researcher was particularly interested in discovering whether persons with the most severe and chronic mental illness would show different rates of involvement with AAINA than others. Due to the heterogeneity among persons with dual diagnosis, different characteristics and experiences might lead to different outcomes within the same sample. Prior studies indicated: (1) that persons with mental illness would be more difficult to engage in substance abuse treatment (of. Liss, 1979; Osher 8 Kofoed, 1989; Panepinto, Higgins, Keane-Dawes, & Smith, 1970) and (2) psychiatric severity was a strong predictor of outcomes in other functioning domains (cf. Gibbs & Flanagan, 1977; McLellan et al., 1983). Findings from this study partially corroborate Osher and Kofoed’s (1989) expectation that persons with severe psychiatric problems would be more difficult to engage in substance abuse treatment. In the current study, participants characterized as high symptom/chronic group were less likely to attend AAINA during the 10-month follow-up, whereas those with fewer symptoms and more acute problems were more likely to attend AAINA and to attend more regularly. Persons with more chronic and/or severe psychiatric problems were more likely to use professional services only, which were primarily outpatient mental health services. While symptom/chronicity was a significant determinant of AAINA attendance, it only accounted for a modest percent of the variance (approximately 103 4.0%). In the path models predicting outcomes from AAINA attendance and symptom/chronicity, symptom/chronicity was no longer a significant predictor. The indirect relationships between symptom/chronicity and functioning at Time 3 were very low or nonexistent. These findings suggest that psychiatric severity and chronicity have some influence on who is likely to attend AAINA. However, the data from the path models indicate that once a connection is made to the self-help group, the severity and chronicity of mental illness are not related to outcomes derived from AAINA participation. Regardless of individual characteristics, persons who were able to attend AAINA more often had fewer alcohol and drug problems, and fewer problems in other life domains. Thus, attendance at AAINA seems to be the more important determinant of outcomes, even for persons with severe and chronic mental illness. Relationship of Service Use to Outcomes Several hypotheses regarding service use and outcome were examined. First, it was hypothesized that persons who attended AAINA regularly after hospitalization would have better outcomes than those who did not attend or who attended sporadically. This hypothesis was supported, but only for alcohol and drug use at Time 3. Persons who attended AAINA had fewer alcohol and drug problems. AAINA attendance was not directly related to better psychological or family/social functioning, nor was it related to the chances of being rehospitalized. This is an important finding, particularly in a dual diagnosis sample. Most of the empirical literature on AAINA attendance and outcomes have been conducted 104 with samples of individuals with singly diagnosed substance abuse problems. Persons with dual diagnoses experience functional deficits in multiple life domains, most notably in their psychological functioning. Whereas previous studies of AAINA have typically utilized alcohol and/or drug use as the only outcome. measures, research with dually diagnosed individuals must incorporate outcomes in other domains in order to reasonably determine the efficacy of a particular intervention. The recovery of persons with dual diagnosis should minimally include improvements in psychological functioning as well as decreases in alcohol and drug use. It is also interesting to note that AAINA attendance was only related to concurrent alcohol and drug problems, but not to prospective problems. The lack of relationship between AAINA attendance and latfi' alcohol or drug problems precludes any causal inferences about the relationship between AAIl\lA service use and sobriety. While it is tempting to infer that AAINA attendance leads to'sobriety, it is equally plausible that persons who already committed to maintaining sobriety gravitate towards AA to support their sobriety (Toumier, 1979). Persons who are actively drinking may not be as motivated to attend AAINA. Previous studies with the current data suggest that motivation (as measured by plans to maintain sobriety) was an important factor related to AAINA attendance and outcome. BootsMiller et al. (1996) found that persons who were more motivated for sobriety when they were discharged from the hospital were subsequently more likely to attend AAINA, and to attain better outcomes. 105 A second important question addressed in this study was the extent to which the benefits of AAINA participation could be augmented with simultaneous involvement in professional services, particulariy those where mental health issues were primarily addressed. Previous authors had suggested that simultaneous involvement in other forms of treatment could obscure the effects of AAINA participation (Emrick, 1987; Thurstin, Alfano, & Nerviano, 1987). Additionally, it was expected that professional treatment programs might be able to address the specific mental health needs (e.g., psychotherapy, medication review) of dually diagnosed persons that would not otherwise be met by attendance to ANNA only. The findings regarding types of service use were contrary to what was expected. Persons who attended AAINA only had the best outcomes. Use of professional services was not related to improvements in any of the functioning domains. In fact, there was some evidence suggesting that persons who used professional services had more problems than those who attended AAINA only. There was a nonsignificant trend for persons attending professional services to report more psychological problems and to be more likely to be rehospitalized. A significant path was observed between use of professional services and worse drug problems. Again, no causal inferences can be drawn from these data as the relationships were observed at a concurrent time point. It is quite plausible that persons with more severe problems were more likely to use professional services, although use of these services did not seem to impact their subsequent functioning (professional service use at Time 3 was not related to improvements in Time 4 106 functioning). Findings and Implications of the Mediated AAINA Treatment Model The results from the structural equation models indicated that there was generally more support for an AAINA treatment model than for the Integrated model. AAINA attendance was not directly related to psychological or family/social functioning, nor to the incidence of rehospitalization. However, there was evidence for a mediated relationship between AAINA attendance and the other outcomes via alcohol/drug use. As suggested by AAINA program theory, individuals who were able to attain sobriety or reduce their alcohol and drug use had fewer problems in the other life domains. This finding was particularly evident in the significant pathway between the number of days of AAINA attendance, Time 3 alcohol problems, and Time 3 psychological problems. Persons who attended AAINA had fewer alcohol problems at Time 3; those with fewer alcohol problems also had fewer psychological problems at Time 3. This finding tends to support prior reports indicating that attainment of abstinence may moderate the relationship between AA attendance and psychological functioning (Akerlind, Hamquist, Elton, & Bjurulf, 1990; Laundergan, 1992). However, this interpretationis made with caution. Given the correlations between the various outcome measures at the same time point (rs range from .07 to .38, with a mean correlation of .24), the significant paths between alcohbl and drug problems and other functioning domains simply suggest that persons with alcohol and drug problems are also likely to have problems in other domains. 107 The findings of this study also indicated that the “dosage” of AAINA was significantly related to outcome. The more frequently person attended AAINA, the better their outcomes were. This is consistent with prior studies (Emrick, 1987; Knouse & Schneider, 1987; McBride, 1991; Thurstin, Alfano, & Nerviano, 1987; Trice & Roman, 1970). In fact, the magnitude of the effect ((3 = -.16) is comparable to that reported in Emrick, Tonigan, Montgomery, and Little’s (1992) meta-analytic review of quantitative studies on AA. These authors reported an average correlation of .19 between AA attendance and drinking outcomes. Limitations of the Current Stu_d_y This study was an improvement over previous investigations in some respects: providing data on a sample of African American males with dual diagnosis, incorporating AAINA program theory into the evaluation, examining other types of service use in addition to AAINA, and assessing outcomes in domains other than alcohol/drug use. Nevertheless, several limitations of this study warrant discussion. First, this study, like prior reports, relied on self-report data. Previous reports have indicated that self-report of sensitive information, such as substance abuse, is biased by under-reporting (Mensch & Kandel, 1988; Polich, 1982; Rouse 8. Kozel, 1985). Also, some authors have expressed concern over the reliance on self-report measures in samples with cognitive or psychological deficits (Drake et al,1990) In addition, some of the key variables used in this study, such as AAINA attendance suffer from selection bias. Due to the fact that participants self- 108 selected to attend AAINA or other services after hospitalization, there is no way to determine conclusively whether persons who attended were different in important (and unmeasured) ways from those who did not attend, and whether these unmeasured differences may have accounted for their outcomes. This is a particular difficulty in conducting research on self-help participation. In order to determine if use of particular services are differentially effective, randomized designs with control groups are necessary. However, prior research indicates that individuals must be personally motivated towards program engagement in order for participation in self-help groups to have any desirable impact. Persons who are randomly assigned or mandated to attend AAINA often do not become sufficiently engaged, and therefore do not receive any benefits from attendance (cf. Ditman, Crawford, Forgy, & Maskowitz, 1967; Edwards et al., 1988; Walsh et al., 1991). The issue of selection bias is an on—going dilemma in research on self-help groups. Perhaps the most serious weakness in this study was the insensitivity of the measures of AAINA participation. The findings of this study are based on noncontinuous data collected at 2-4 month intervals. In order to determine the efficacy of AAINA, more direct and specific measures of engagement are needed. For instance, prior studies indicate that the quality of AAINA participation may be as important or more important than the quantity of attendance. Maxwell (1984) indicated that in order for persons to derive benefit from AA, they must engage themselves in the fellowship aspects of recovery. This fellowship includes forming relationships with sponsors and other group members, giving testimonials about 109 their own recovery, and “participating in open, honest sharing, and caring interaction with fellow members” (Maxwell, 1984, p. 155). The current study did not assess these qualitative aspects of AAINA involvement. Additionally, engagement in 12-step work and self-realization is an important aspect of AAINA involvement which was not measured in this study. Without these measures, it is impossible to determine how well participants in this study were actually “working" the AA program. Conclusions & Implications Despite its limitations, this study suggests some interesting implications for mental health policy and future research. The participants in this study with the most severe psychiatric problems and the longest histories of psychiatric hospitalization were less likely to engage in self-help programs following ho'spitalization, but were more likely to continue using mental health services. Use of mental health services seemed to have some positive relationship with alcohol problems, but the relationship was very small and there was no positive relationship with drug problems or problems in any of the other functioning domains. These findings indicate a disturbing trend, suggesting that persons with chronic and severe psychiatric problems were not receiving much support for their sobriety once they were discharged from the hospital. Given the fact that most of the participants in this study were not receiving outpatient substance abuse services, nor was the use of outpatient mental health services significantly related to improvements in alcohol and drug use, AAINA attendance seemed to be the only 110 service which was useful in helping participants continue to receive support for and maintain their sobriety. More research is needed to determine the impact of professional mental health services when used in combination with AAINA. In clinical practice, persons with substance abuse problems are often encouraged to seek individual counseling while attending self-help groups. It is evident that persons with dual diagnosis need to be connected with substance abuse and/or dual diagnosis networks in order to maintain their sobriety. This study suggests that this combination of services is more beneficial than no service use, at least for improvement in alcohol problems. Therefore, mental health professionals may serve an important role in helping persons with substance abuse and/or psychological problems to access and benefit from self-help groups. For persons with long histories of mental health problems and psychiatric hospitalization, the mental health services network may be a more familiar and easily assessable resource. Mental health service providers could be instrumental in implementing linkage programs to help those who would not otherwise become engaged with self-help and sobriety support programs. If persons with severe and chronic mental illness are helped in the process of engagement, this study’s findings suggest that they will receive the benefits of sobriety (or at least be able to reduce their alcohol and drug use). Prior work by Minkoff (1989) suggests that with special preparation prior to hospital discharge, persons with dual diagnosis were able to successfully engage in AAINA groups. Clients receiving dual diagnosis 1 1 1 treatment. were prepared for AAINA attendance via a structured program including social skills training, linkage with other AAINA members, and trial attendance at selected meetings thought to be most suitable for persons with dual diagnosis. The work of Minkoff (1989) and others is promising, however, there is a lack of empirical data to document the efficacy of these program initiatives. Mm Directions The findings of this study suggest several promising avenues for future research on AAINA attendance among persons with dual diagnosis. A few prior studies have suggested that attainment of sobriety moderates the relationship between AAINA attendance and psychological functioning. This study partly corroborated prior efforts, however, the findings were only identified cross- sectionally. No causal inferences could be made based on findings from the current data. There is a need for prospective longitudinal studies to explicate the relationship between psychological distress and substance abuse, and how these are related to AAINA attendance. While the current study indicated that persons who attended AAINA more regularly attained better outcomes, the specific aspects of the AAINA program related to positive outcome could not be identified. Future research should address the following questions: Which aspects of the AAINA program are most helpful to persons with dual diagnosis? In what ways do persons with dual diagnosis ”work" the AAINA program? And what intrapersonal and group-related factors are related to their successful engagement in the program? LIST OF REFERENCES LIST OF REFERENCES Alcoholics Anonymous (1952). Twelve steps and twelve tra_dition§. NY: Alcoholics Anonymous World Services. Alcoholics Anonymous (1975). Living sober. NY: Alcoholics Anonymous World Services. Alcoholics Anonymous (1976). flmholiwjnonvmous: The story of how many thousands of men and women have recovered: from alcoholism (3rd Ed.). NY: Alcoholics Anonymous Worid Services. Alcoholics Anonymous (1989). Apalvsis cflhe 1986 member survey. (Available from General Service Office, 468 Park Ave., New York, NY. 10163). Akerlind, l., Homquist, J. 0., Elton, M., 8 Bjurulf, P. (1990). Overall functioning and criteria of progress in rehabilitation of alcohol abusers: Longitudinal analyses of change. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 14(6), 856- 862. Alford, G. S. (1980). Alcoholics Anonymous: An empirical outcome study. A_d_<_lictive Behaviors, 5, 359-370. Bartalos, M. K. (1992). Illness, professional caregivers, and self-helpers. In A. H. Katz et al. (Eds) Self-help: Concepts and applications. Philadelphia: The Charles Press, Publishers. Bateman, N. l. & Petersen, D. M. (1971). Variables related to outcome of treatment for hospitalized alcoholics. lntemational Journal of the Addictions, 6(2), 215-224. Bebbington, P. E. (1976). The efficacy of Alcoholics Anonymous: The elusiveness of hard data. British Journal of Psychiatpz, 128, 572-580. Belasco, J. A. (1971). The criterion question revisited. British Journal of Addiction, §_6_, 39-44. 112 113 Bentler, P. M. (1995). EQS structural eguations program manual. Encino, CA: Multivariate Software, Inc. ' BootsMiller, B. J., Jordan, L. C., Davidson, W. S., 8 Herman, S. E.,(1996). An evaluation of a specialized dual disorder treatment tomersons with mental illness and substance abuse: flects of tr_eatment on service use and outcomes. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Michigan State University, East Lansing. Borman, L. D. (1992). Introduction: Self-helplmutual aid groups in strategies for health. In A. H. Katz et al. (Eds.) Self-help: Concepts and applications. Philadelphia: The Charles Press, Publishers. ~ Boscarino, J. (1980). Factors related to "stable" and "unstable" affiliation with Alcoholics Anonymous. lntemational Journal of the Adaictiona, 15(6), 839-848. Boume, P. G. 8 Fox, R. (1973). Alcoholism: Progress in research and treatment. New York: Academic Press. Bradley, A. M. (1988). Keep coming back: A case for a valuation of Alcoholics Anonymous. Alcohol Health and Research Worig, 12(3), 192-199. Bromet, E.: Moos, R.; Bliss, F.; 8 Wuthmann, C. (1977). Posttreatment functioning of alcoholic patients: Its relation to program participation. Journal of Consulting and Cliniaal Psychology, 45(5), 829-842. Brown, B. 8.; Joe, G. W.; 8 Thompson, P. (1985). Minority group status and treatment retention. The lntemational Jourpal of the Ad_dictions. 2_0, 319-335. Buxton, M. E., Smith, D. E., 8 Seymour, R. B. (1987). Spirituality and other points of resistance to the 12-step recovery process. Journal of Psychoactive Drugs, 19(3), 275-286. Canter, F. M. (1966). Personality factors related to participation in treatment by hospitalized alcoholics. Journal of Clinifil Psychology, 22(2), 114-116. Chacko, R. C., Adams, G. L., 8 Gomez, E. (1985). The care of the chronic mental patients: A historical perspective. In R. C. Chacko (Ed.), The chronic mental patient in a community context. Washington, D. C.: American Psychiatric Press. Chen, H. (1990). Theory-driven evaluations. Newbury Park: Sage. Denzin, N. K. (1987). The recovering alcoholic. Newbury Park: Sage Publications. ‘ 114 Derogatis, L., Lipman, R. 8 Covi L. (1973). The SOL-90: An outpatient rating scale. PsvchoLhamcology Bulletin, _9_, 13-29. DiClemente, (1993). Alcoholics Anonymous and the structure of change. In B. S. McCrady 8 W. R. Miller (Eds) Research on Alcoholics Anonymotg Opportunities and alternatives. New answick: Rutgers Center of Alcohol Studies. Ditman, K. 8.; Crawford, G. G.; Forgy, E. W.; Moskowitz, H.; 8 MacAndrew, C. (1967). A controlled experiment on the use of court probations for dmnk arrests. American Journal of Psychiatpy, 124(2), 161-163. Drake, R. E., Osher, F. c., Noordsy, o. L., Hurlbut, s. c., Teague, G. B. & Beaudett, M. S. (1990). Diagnosis of alcohol use disorders in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 1_6_, 5767. Edwards, 6.; Orford, J.; Egert, S.; Guthrie, S.; Hawker, A.; Hensman, C.; Mitcheson, M.; Oppenheimer, E.; 8 Taylor, C. (1977). Alcoholism: A controlled trial of "treatment" and "advice." Joumal of Sttflies on Alcohol. 38(5), 1004-1031. Elal-Lawrence, G.; Slade, P. D.; 8 Dewey, M. E. (1987). Treatment and follow-up variables discriminating abstainers, controlled drinkers and relapsers. Journal of Stugies on Alcohol. 48(1). 39-46. Elal-Lawrence, G.; Slade, P. D.; 8 Dewey, M. E. (1986). Predictors of outcome type in treated problem drinkers. Journal of Stu_dies on Alcohol. 47(1). 41- 47. Emener, W. G. 8 Dickman, J. F. (1992). Significant demographic characteristics of persons recovering from alcoholism and other drugs. Journal of Rehabilitation Counseling, 23(1), 3-17. ' Emrick, C. D. (1987). Alcoholics Anonymous: Affiliation process and effectiveness as treatment. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Reseaycfl, 1_1, 416-423. Emrick C. D. (1989). Alcoholics Anonymous: Emerging concepts. In M. Galanter (Ed) Recent gevelopments in alcoholism. Vol. 7. New York: Plenum Press. Emrick, C. D., Tonigan, J. 8., Montgomery, H., 8 Little, L. (1993). Alcoholics Anonymous: What is currently known? In B. S. McCrady 8 W. R. Miller (Eds.) Research on Alcoholics Anonymous: Opportunities and alternatives (pp. 41-76). New Brunswick: Rutgers Center of Alcohol Studies. 115 Fainrveather, G. W., Sanders, D. H., Maynard, H., Cressler, D. L., 8 Bleck, D. S. (1969). Community life for the mentafl ill: An altema_tive to institutional care. Chicago: Aldine Publishing Company. Faulkner, W.; Sandage, D.; 8 Maguire, B. (1988). The disease concept of alcoholism: The persistence of an outmoded scientific paradigm. Deviant Behavior, _9_, 317-332. Francis, D. J., Fletcher, J. M., Stuebing, K K., Davidson, K. C., 8 Thompson, NM. (1991). Analysis of change: Modeling individual growth. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 59(1), 27-37. Gibbs 8 Flanagan (1977). Prognostic indicators of alcoholism treatment outcome. lntemational Journal of the Ad_dictions. 12(8), 1097-1141. Glaser, F. B., 8 Ogbome, A. C. (1982). Does A.A. really work? British Journal of Acfiiction. fl, 123-129. Gregson, R. A. M. 8 Taylor, G. M. (1977). Prediction of relapse in men alcoholics. Journal of Strflies on Alcohol. 38(9), 1749-1760. Haberrnan, P. W. (1966). Factors related to increased sobriety in group therapy with alcoholics. Mmal of Clinical Psychology, 22(2), 229-235. Hanson, B. (1985). Drug treatment effectiveness: The case of racial minorities in America - Some research questions and proposals. The lntemational Journal of the Addictions, _2_Q, 99-137. Heather, N.; Rollnick, S.; 8 WInton, M. (1983). A comparison of objective measures of alcohol dependence as predictors of relapse following treatment. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, _2_2, 11-17. Hedrick, H. L., lsenberg, D. H., 8 Martini, C. J. M. (1992). Self-help groups: Empowerment through policy and partnerships. In A. H. Katz et al. (Eds.) Self- help: Concepts and applications. Philadelphia: The Charies Press, Publishers. Heller, K. (1979). The effects of social support: prevention and treatment implications. In A. P. Goldstein 8 F. H. Kanfer (Eds.), Maximizing treatment gains: Transfer enhancement in psychotherapy (pp. 87-103. NY: Academic Press. Heller, K 8 Swindle, R. W. (1983). Social networks, perceived social support, and coping with stress. In R. D. Felner etal. (Eds. ) Preventive psychology: Theory research and practice. NY: Pergamon. 116 Hellerstein, D. J. 8 Meehan, B. (1987). Outpatient group therapy for schizophrenic substance abusers. American Journal of Psychiatpy, fl, 1337-1339. Herman, S. E., BootsMiller, B. J., Jordan, L. C., Mowbray, C. T., Brown, W. G., Deiz, N., Bandla, H., Solomon, M., 8 Green, P. (1996). Immediate outcomes of substance use treatment within a state psychiatric hospital. Manuscript submitted for publication. Hoffman, N. G. 8 Overall, P. B. (1978). Factor structure of the SCL-90 in a psychiatric population. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 46(6), 1187- 1 191 . Hoffman, N. G.; Harrisbn, P. A.; 8 Belille, C. A. (1983). Alcoholics Anonymous after treatment: Attendance and abstinence. The lntemational Journal of the Ad_dictions. 18(3), 311-318. Howell, D. C. (1987). %tisticamethofids for psychology (Second Edition). Boston: Duxbury Press. Humphreys, K. 8 Woods, M. D. (1993). Researching mutual help group participation in a segregation society. The Journal of Applied Behavioaa! Sciences, 29(2), 181-201. Institute of Medicine (1990). Broadening the base of treatment for alcohol problems. Washington, DC: National Academy Press. Kanas (1982). Alcoholism and group psychotherapy. In E. M. Pattison 8 E. Kaufman (Eds) Encyclopedic handbook of alcoholism (pp. 1011-1021). NY: Gardner Press. Kay, S. R., Kalathara, M., 8 Meinzer, A. E. (1989). Diagnostic and behavioral characteristics of psychiatric patients who abuse substances. Hospital and Community Psychiatg, 19, 1062-1064. Kazdin, A. E. 8 leon, T. (1978). Criteria for evaluating psychotherapy. Archives of General Psychiatry 35 407-415. Kenny, D. A. (1979). Correlation and causality. NY: WIIey Press. King, L.M. (1983). Alcoholism: Studies regarding Black Americans- 1977- 1980. In T.D. Watts 8 R. Wright (Eds), Black alcoholism: Toward a pomprehensive understanding. Springfield: Charles C. Thomas Publisher. Knight, B., Wollert, R. W., Levy, L. H., Frame, C. L., 8 Padgett, V. P. 117 (1980). Self-help groups: The members’ perspectives. American Journal of Community Psycholouy, a, 53-65. Knouse, V. H. 8 Schneider, H. G. (1987). Recovering alcoholics: Personality and aftercare factors. Psychological Reports, §_1_, 595-601. Korte, C. (1978). Helpfulness in the urban environment. In A. Baum, J. E. Singer, 8 S. Valins (Eds), Advances in environmentaipsycholomr. Vol 1. Tha urban environment. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Eribaum. Kurtz, E. (1993). Research on Alcoholics Anonymous: The historical context. In B. S. McCrady 8 W. R. Miller (Eds), Research on Alcoholics Anonymous: Opportunities and alternatives. New Brunswick: Rutgers Center of Alcohol Studies. Laundergan, J. C. (1993). How could studies of Alcoholics Anonymous be designed? Evaluation within treatment contexts. In B. S. McCrady 8 W. R. Miller (Eds) Research on Alcoholics Anonymous: Opportunities and altematives. New Brunswick: Rutgers Center of Alcohol Studies. Leiberrnan, M. A. 8 Borman, L. D. (1979). Self-help groups for coping with crisis. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass Publishers. Leiberrnan, M. A. 8 Snowden, L. R. (1993). Problems in assessing prevalence and membership characteristics of self-help group participants. Iaa Journal of Applied Behavioral Scienc_e, 29(2), 166-180. Liss, R. A. (1979). The need for homogeneity and consistency. In D. J. Ottenberg, J.F. X. Carroll, 8 C. Bolognese (Eds) Treating mixeupsychotic drug- addicted and alcoholic Etienta. Eagleville, PA: Eagleville Hospital and Rehabilitation Center. Machell, D. F. (1989). Alcoholics Anonymous: A wonderful medication with some possible side effects. Journal of Alcohol and Drug Education, 34(3), 80-84. Machell, D. F. (1992). A psychological rationale in support of the Alcoholics Anonymous' concept of fellowship. Journal of Alcohol and Drug Education, 37(3), 1-16. Maisto, S. A., O'Farrell, T. J., McKay, J. R., Connors, G. J., 8 Pelcovits, M. (1989). Factors in maintaining sobriety following alcohol treatment. Alcoholism Treatment uarterl ,6(3I4), 143-150. Maxwell, M. A. (1984). The Alcoholics Anonymous exparience. NY: 118 McGraw-Hill Book Company. McBride, J. L. (1991). Abstinence among members of Alcoholics Anonymous. Alcoholism Treatment Quarterly. 8(1), 113-121. McCrady, B. S. 8 Irvine, S. (1989). Self-help Groups. In R. K. Hester 8 W. R. Miller (Eds), Handbook of alcoholism treatment approacheszuEffectjya alternatives. (pp. 153-169). NY: Pergamon Press. McLatchie, B. H 8 Lomp, K. G. E. (1988). Alcoholics Anonymous affiliation and treatment outcome among a clinical sample of problem drinkers. American Journal of Drug anu Alcohol Abuse, 309-324. McLellan, A. T., Luborsky, L., Woody, G. E., O’Brien, C. P., 8 Druley, K. A. (1983). Predicting response to alcoholic and drug abuse treatments. Archives of GenereiLDsychiatry. fl, 620-625. McLellan, A. T., Luborsky, L., Woody, G. E., 8 O’Brien, C. P. (1980). An improved diagnostic evaluation instrument for substance abuse patients: The Addiction Severity Index. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 3&8, 26-33. Mensch, B. S. 8 Kandel, D. B. (1988). Underreporting of substance use in a national longitudinal youth cohort. who Opinion Quarterly, Q, 100-124. Miller, W. R. 8 Hester, R. K. (1980). Treating the problem drinker. Modern approaches. In W. R. Miller (Ed), The addictive behaviors: Treatment of alcoholism, drug abuse, smoking, and obesity (pp.11-141). NY: Pergamon Press. Miller, W. R. 8 Hester, R. K. (1986). Matching problem drinkers with optimal treatments. In W. R. Miller 8 N. Heather (Eds), Treating addictive behaviors: Process of changa (pp.175-203). NY: Plenum Press. Miller, W. R. 8 McCrady, B. S. (1993). The importance of research on Alcoholics Anonymous. In B. S. McCrady 8 W. R. Miller (Eds), Research on Alcoholics Anonymous: Opportunities and alternatives. New Brunswick: RUtgers Center of Alcohol Studies. Minkoff, K. (1989). An integrated treatment model for dual diagnosis of psychosis and addiction. Hospital and Community Psychiatgy, 519, 1031-1036. Minkoff, K. 8 Drake, R. E. (1991). Dual diagnosis of major mental illness and substance disorder. New Directions for MentarlHaalth Services, _5_0_, 3-12. Moos, R. H., Finney, J., 8 Maude-Griffin, P. (1993). The social climate of 119 self-help and mutual support groups: Assessing group implementatiOn, process, and outcome. In B. S. McCrady 8 W. R. Miller (Eds), Research on Alcoholics Anonymous: Opportunities and alternatives. New Brunswick: Rutgers Center of Alcohol Studies. Mueser, K. T., Bellack, A. S., 8 Blanchard, J.J. (1992). Comorbidity of schizophrenia and substance abuse: Implications for treatment. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psycholggy, 65), 845-856. Nguyen, T. D., Atikisson, C. C., 8 Stegner, B. L. (1984). Assessment of patient satisfaction. Evaluation and Program Planning, _6_, 299-314. Norris, J. L. (1978). Prevention of chronicity in alcoholism. Psychiatric Annals, 8(11), 48-53. Norusis, M. J. (1993). SPSS for windows. Chicago: SPSS, Inc. O'Farrell, T.J., Connors, G. J., 8 Upper, D. (1983). Addictive behaviors among hospitalized psychiatric patients. _Ad_dictive Befiviors. 8, 329-333. Ogbome, A. C. (1989). Some limitations of Alcoholics Anonymous. _In Marc Galanter (Ed) Recengevelopments in_a_lcoholism Vol.7 (pp. 55-65). NY: Plenum Press. Ogbome, A. C. 8 Bomet, A. (1982). Abstinence and abusive drinking among affiliates of Alcoholics Anonymous: Are these the only alternatives? Ad_dictive Behaviors. 1, 199-202. Ogbome, A. C. 8 Glaser, F. B. (1981). Characteristics of affiliates of Alcoholics Anonymous: A review of the literature. Journal of Stu_dies on Alcohol. 42(7), 661 -675. O'Leary, M. R., Calsyn, D. A., Haddock, D. L., 8 Freeman, C. W. (1980). Differential alcohol use patterns and personality traits among three Alcoholics Anonymous attendance level groups: Further considerations of the affiliation profile. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 2 135-144. Osher, F. C. 8 Kofoed, L. L. (1989). Treatment of patients with psychiatric and psychoactive substance abuse disorders. Hospital and Community Psychiatgy, up, 1025-1030. Otteson, J. P. (1979). Curative caring: The use of buddy groups with chronic schizophrenics. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 47(3), 649-651. 120 Panepinto, W. C., Higgins, M. J., Keane-Dawes, W. Y., Smith, D. (1970). Underlying psychotic diagnoses as an indicator of participation in alcoholism therapy. Quarterly Journal of StLulies on Alcohol. 3_1, 950-956. Peele, S. (1990). Research issues in assessing addiction treatment efficacy: How cost effective are Alcoholics Anonymous and private treatment centers? Drug and Alcohol Dependence, g, 179-182. Polich, J. M. (1982). The validity of self-reports in alcoholism research. Addictive Behaviors, 1, 123-132. Polich, J. M.; Armor, D. J.; 8 Braiker, H. B. (1980). The course of alcoholism: Four years after treatment. Santa Monica: Rand. Powell, T. J. (1993). Self-help research and policy issues. The Journal of Applied Behavioral Science. 29(2), 151-165. Prochaska, J. O., DeCIemente, C. C., 8 Norcross, J. C. (1992). In search of how people change. American Psychologis_t, 47(9), 1102-1114. Rather, B. C. 8 Sherman, M. F. (1989). Relationship between alcohol expectancies and length of abstinence among Alcoholics Anonymous members. Addictive Behaviors, 15, 531-536. Ribisl, K. M. (1995). A longitudinal study of the personal networks of individuals receivinLtreatment for mental illness and substance abuse problems. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Michigan State University, East Lansing. Rouse, B. A., Kozel, N. J., 8 Richards, L. G. (1985). Self-report methogs of estimating drug use: Meeting current challenges to validity. NIDA Research monograph 57 (DHHS Publication No. (ADM) 85-1402). Rockville, MD: U. S. Government Printing Office. Sanchez-Craig, M., Annis, H. M., Bomet, A. R., 8 MacDonald, K. R. (1984). Random assignment to abstinence and controlled drinking: Evaluation of a cognitive-behavioral program for problem drinkers. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, gag), 390-403. Scott, E. M. (1986). Beyond A.A. lntemational Journal of Offender Therapy and Comparative Criminology, 30(1), 69-75. Seixas, F. A., Washbum, S., 8 Eisen, S. V. (1988). Alcoholism, Alcoholics Anonymous attendance, and outcome in a prison system. American Journal of Drug and Alcohol Abuse, 14(4), 515-524. 121 Selzer, M. L. (1971). The Michigan Alcoholism Screening Test: The quest for a new diagnostic instmment. American Journal of Psychiatpy, 1_2_7, 1653-1658. Smart, R. 6., Mann, R. E., 8 Anglin, L. (1989). Decreases in alcohol problems and increased Alcoholics Anonymous membership. British Journal of Addiction, 84(5), 507-513. Stone, W. N. (1991). Treatment of the chronically mentally ill: An opportunity for the group therapist. lntemational Journal of Group Psychotherapy, flu), 11-22. Swift, CF. 8 Beverly, S. (1985). The utilization of Ministers as Alcohol Counselors and Educators: Increasing prevention and treatment resources in the black community. In R. Wright 8 TD. Watts (Eds) Prevention of Black alcoholism. Springfield: Charles C. Thomas Publisher. Swindle, R. W., Cronkite, R. C., 8 Moos, R. H. (1989). Life stressors, social resources, coping, and the 4-year course of unipolar depression. Journal of Manna) Psychology, 98(4), 468-477. Thompson, T. 8 Simmons-Cooper, C. (1988). Chemical dependency treatment and Black adolescents. The Journal of Drug Issues, Ju, pp.21-31. Thurstin, A. H., Alfano, A. M., 8 Nerviano, V. J. (1987). The efficacy of AA attendance for aftercare of inpatient alcoholics: Some follow-up data. lntemational Journal of the Acflictiona, 22(11), 1083-1090. Tomsovic, M. (1970). A follow-up study of discharged alcoholics. Hospital app Community Psychiauy, 21(3), 94-97. Toumier, R. E. (1979). Alcoholics Anonymous as treatment and as ideology. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 40(3). 230-239. Trice, H. M. 8 Roman, R. M. (1970). Sociopsychological predictors of affiliation with Alcoholics Anonymous. Social Psychiatm, an), 51-59. Vaillant, G. E. (1983). The natural histogy of alcoholism. Cambridge: Harvard University Press. Walsh et al. (1991). A randomized trail of treatment options for alcohol- abusing workers. New England Journal of Medicine, 2Q. 775-782. Westerrneyer, J. (1981). Research on treatment of drinking problems: Importance of cultural factors. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, Suppl. 9, 44-57. Westerrneyer, J. 8 Neider, J. (1988). Social networks and psychopathology 122 among substance abusers. American Journal of Psychiatgy, 145(10), 1265-1269. erkey, W. W. (1986). The influence of Alcoholics Anonymous on alcoholism treatment. Employee Assistance Quarteriy, 1(5), 1-17. WIIson, W. H., Diamond, R. J., and Factor, R. M. (1990). The chronically mentally ill: Group treatment for individuals with schizophrenia. Cpmmunity Mental Health Journal, 26(4), 361-372. Yalom, I. D. (1985). The theopy and practice of group psychotherapy. (3rd edition). Basic Books. Zweben, J. E. and Smith, D. E. (1987). Can the patient on medication be sent to 12-step programs? Journal of Psychoactive Drugs, 2_1, 221-228 APPENDIX 123 338.: .282 a... <22... 33:... 33.. 83.2 ”.032 3.3.2.: .3. 83: \. 32.63 .3: m nan—a8 _2—onw—< V ,Wgw /fl :5 <22... m 3:. 33.3 3mg m 32.—ova 33w 3.8.... m 3. . 8 .S.\fl/ 5.. M /.. /mm. 124 .532.— 25 ...: <22}. .58... 55% .352 ".232 3:33 .~.< 23... fluaauim .3.me ha. 2:033.“ «En v 08:. > 8 2:23 .3: 930 m gnaw :5 <75}. m 3:. gum. 2.863 sow 8.- / «€333.23 _ m 25w 3. {/22 128 m 33%... _2§< ...: E33 8:232 <75} ”.33: 33.3.: .e-< 83E genuxum .3me m .332 33.2 V 2a...“ 38%,» flaw 333% 35:38.4 <23}. 3853 39w .38.... m 2... no .3. )\ .3039. / . .w «a o.— 129 .535 «an ...: as...“ 838.2 <22). "33: 323.: .32 23E bu. \ 3:853 32g «Eu—cam nan— ¢ 08:. ) a 9 N 5:: 33¢; __ mg . 882.82 423‘ oon 2.- / .935 . 38:63 33 «an m 3:. 8 / «a. .338.— 233303 2. 83.: 83.8.3 <23). ”.332 3:93 .a.< 2.3... 0 3 \3. 3.2.63 .32 .8032.— 193—one“: * earn. \1/ 33 u. m 53.; aim .— .mw . 0282.33. <2): oav 3.0382 /\ / . 33393 33 723.39»; M 26,—. «a o4 131 .535 3835a: _a. as...” 838.2 <2)? ”.35: 33.2: «i 83E \ma. finch—Dim Ada m, .8038 335m . g... 8.8456 Adm—fig noun—30:4. no 2:33 ..ng 38303 32w E03; 3. m. 352.82 56: «3333923 m 3:. 133 «8038.— _2_oo_< v3 25 vacuum mo 093. ”—302 33333 ._ T< oBuE ma. 3. / F as £09.30“ 1H3uu0wem h 32.—5 a. 34002 c 06E. fl such—oim A3: > «axiom 23 m. .3. inauuououm + 5433 A sum .— .EW 2533 co.— _2_§< m 3:. >15 <29}. 030.39% Bo.— / ¢¢. “Eu—poi man can on: 3:35 we 2.3. ”332 3.23:: .224. 2:»; ma. \ 2.5 A . \ 323% 2550.35 25:83 .35 134 ”£932.— wEQ v 05:. a . 32:; .3 :55985 + row. mac—98m _aommofinommm m 2.5. no. we. 3.. :1. _o. mEoEoE _uomwfloaummm v5 3.: 333m mo 33. ”—232 33535 .m_-< EsmE no. \ >25 mootcom 35:80.35 f «335m 323%... + on. 8. 323.5 9 $333.35 + <23}. A 32:35 .35 —°. / «Bozo; cc.— EoomEES m as; S 2. 3:0 84 ...: - r g. conun_§_%o:om EB 83 830m mo 33. “.252 33%an .m _-< 2:me m¢. a \ moomtom 12833.35 2.33me as: vo. :ouafifiamoaom E . 7 .. 3 v 05E. mm. vo. moomiom _., mm.» o :32 a .5 + om. . :5 <22: m ”a: i 2:23 :5 saga—232.3“ 2532a :28: m 08:. A 0:: mo. r m ..F a. 3. anoEExm Bog mo. 3. 1 4 1 mac—no:— Eflmo—oaommm EB 8088 unguaou< .3. 5oz; . 33$; .33.— 35222 5:: 8 - no. u 82 no; 80305 3:32 a A on: :35 zaz m ”a: 8. f m ..F ma. no... 3:8: 036 33 3. 2: Engaging»: as 533.. 85232 <2)? H3.32 2053; ..bm. 58::— . oSofixm .35 0233.32 *hN a0 9 ...mv. moatom . 8:336 :93 33:33; + ao _m. 323$ . 52:35 + $55. 9:23 3: «2.. mEofioE 8030.5 .282 _soomizeé m 25 n 2:; o3 . ac. 3 mo 4/ 4/ . ‘ 8 . *ON . EGO *NN. 9 on. moetom t .. $333.35 +