4‘1 4 V . . ...-25 a... . V Rearinx. . .. V . , V . ...Vrflmfimfiz , .1 . .. .... .3? .. r. wawfi . V «93.? a: xgfib’iaféfi. ' .33 ~ “3 ’& a: «mm. «.9 V 2 an”. .31“; w «n was 3.4.? ."r r. .u: v' .....fiw. nan . . , . .. r4 ., V :53 “gm...” 5a....“ , V m _ . th:»4mfiu u . .4. ,o . «WP... 2-. V . é a: 3.3121,: . V . V '0 r. .n . L.>*!f‘l . ‘ 1AM . . . 9. ..v.. ... . V V V . V . . . . . v . dvnl . , V , . . » L21... V .3 . , V , . V . . ....“ 3...”an .. V , . V _ .. .. V... , . , V , a“. :r. ..r a , V 3% V; Kama, r: . no . .... 71w. .5. .. 9J9.“ ... , sir .. 3 at 51.. .;...at.»..;. r: . .. , an .V V , 333%.,ch . «u , . Eu... ...... V. V V . . i.» . _ V . _ V V . . . .. . ”N‘cmanflv , V . «fit; {.3 . , , . 4.4.... ... n1. “.5...an . 0- A ER. 3 y. r ‘ r. (\l V N ‘.~ '1‘. . , Avian“: . .5... u. . . , 21.22.»: , .... ...»..uxr‘! . we . V . , . . .‘V . ... VVhthifiv. R... 3. & ...: Pi. ... .VHW r. ‘ . I 4:... .2... . 7 .3... 1:; .u. . .V...: 2. 3 . . I V. 1 .V v .5 :5: u . V I:=£.1,Hr V 33.33 THESIS Zocl LIBRARY Michigan State University This is to certify that the dissertation entitled PSYCHOLOGICAL INVESTIGATIONS OF EDUCATIONAL EXPLORATIONS WITH TECHNOLOGY: UNDERSTANDING WHAT MAKES A 'GOOD INNOVATION' presented by RICHARD EUGENE FERDIG has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for PH . D . EDUCATI ONAL P SYCHOLOGY degree in ”470.920,... / Major professor Date 4V, tawlM/JZ. 2000 MS U i: an Affirmative Action/Equal Opportunity Institution 042771 PLACE IN RETURN BOX to remove this checkout from your record. TO AVOID FINES return on or before date due. MAY BE RECALLED with earlier due date if requested. DATE DUE DATE DUE ' DATE DUE JULBP 8 2862 IHWZW 11/00 eJCIRC/DaioDmpGS-p.“ PSYCHOLOGICAL INVESTIGATIONS OF EDUCATIONAL EXPLORATIONS WITH TECHNOLOGY: UNDERSTANDING WHAT MAKES A ‘GOOD INNOVATION’ By Richard Eugene Ferdig A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Counseling, Educational Psychology, and Special Education 2000 ABSTRACT PSYCHOLOGICAL INVESTIGATIONS OF EDUCATIONAL EXPLORATIONS WITH TECHNOLOGY: UNDERSTANDING WHAT MAKES A “GOOD INNOVATION’ By Richard Eugene Ferdig In this study, I set out to answer the question, “What makes a good technological innovation?”-—an important mandate for our young field of educational technology. Drawing on an exploration of an innovation entitled, ‘Got Milk?’, as well as an in-depth literature review, I establish that a good innovation is one that consists of three “P's”: pedagogy, people, and performance. I argue that this deep psychological approach helps us establish a more multi-layered and complete understanding of the impact of technology innovations. In working to establish this model of a good innovation, and thus to learn more about the participants working with the innovation, I adapted an interview protocol from Dan McAdams (McAdams, 1995) that leads a person through the telling of their story. This narrative approach, initially used as a methodology to understand educational technology implementation, was used by the teachers and students to further develop their teliographies regarding life, teaching, and teaching with technology. Understanding the elements of a good innovation is an important task for our young field. It allows US to revisit what we already know, ground what we are currently working on, and guide our future endeavors. The unintended outcome of the positive use of the narrative methodology is also important as it offers the potential for a new approach to teaching teachers about technology. Implications for both of these points are drawn for teachers, developers, and teacher educators. Copyright by RICHARD EUGENE FERDIG 2000 DEDICATION In the fall of 2000, just as the proverbial light at the end of the dissertation tunnel was in sight, my grandmother passed away. I was reminded of her passing as | sat in my living room watching a movie (Mom would call it procrastinating). A character in the movie pondered the decisions she made in life: “I have the sneaking suspicion that I chose the wrong path. For the past ten years, I have struggled to achieve what I thought was most important in life. Now that I have reached those goals and achieved what I held most dear, I wonder what I missed along the way. I wonder if what I pursued really has any value.” I do believe that the path I have chosen has value—even if it meant not having more time with my loved ones. I have seen it in the faces of the teachers and students I have had the opportunity to share my education with. However, I must confess—there are still moments when I feel the need to be apologetic for being selfish with my time and my studies. Therefore, I dedicate this dissertation to my family and friends who were so patient with me over the last ten years from BA. to MA. to PhD. Thank you for your sacrifice and support as I came to better know and understand myself and the world around me. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS I am not sure when a person ever officially starts the process that leads to a dissertation. I suppose the impetus to attend and finish graduate school came from three sources: my faith, my family and my academic mentors. My faith got me through the darkest hour—times when I questioned who I was, what I did, and why I did it. I praise the Lord that l have fought the good fight, I have finished the race, and I have kept the faith. My Mom and Dad, the people who planted the seed of faith, were supportive and nurturing in multiple ways. They ensured a home visit was filled with good food, opportunities for discussion, and gentle probing about the finish line. Even though my choices in life were new terrain for them, they acted interested as I explained what ‘pedagogy’ meant. I do not know if I will ever be able to fully explain the desire to attend graduate school; nonetheless, they cared because they knew I did. Other family and friends were also self-less during this time. My sister, brother-in-Iaw, and niece gave me time to unwind and roof their house in the process. My grandparents, aunt and uncle, and friend ‘P’ bought me a surprise package of external motivation towards the end of the campaign. And friends like Pawel, C-Lyn, Dan, Mike, and Ron kept calling even when I did not have time to return their calls. To reiterate what I stated in my dedication, my family and friends gave me the opportunity to be selfish—something I needed from them in order to attain this important goal. vi Although the support of my family and friends was essential, I would not have completed this dissertation without the support of my academic mentors. I owe a debt of gratitude to 0.. former professor at Calvin College where I completed my BA. I first learned about the potential of narrative in his cognitive psychology course. John was willing to take some pedagogical risks and they paid off for the students who were willing to story their life. In a chance meeting later at Michigan State, he urged me to continue devoting my life to what I valued. Dick McLeod and Leighton Price were two important faculty members as l transitioned to Masters study at Michigan State. I had temporarily set aside my interest in narrative to further explore my fascination with technology. Dick and Leighton were two of the first people to ever show me how to care about teachers and students in Classes, regardless of the subject you were teaching. They both provided opportunities to get involved in the right things at the right time. I am grateful to both of them for their continued support. In 1996, armed with strong interests and experiences in both technology and narrative, I began the final leg of the journey that led to this dissertation. There are too many names for me to list all of the people that have made this dissertation possible. However, I want to s ecifically thank six people. I became friends with @ an P. David Pearson as l was leaving Michigan State in 1997. I had convinced myself that the grass was greener on the other side of the fence. I was planning on attending a different university to focus my studies on narrative and the development of the child. King Beach helped me realize that vii sometimes moving places only means moving sideways. Once I decided that King was right, P. David Pearson took me under his wing and showed me what it meant to be an academic who made a difference. He was instrumental in first getting me published and in the completion of this final degree. In the summer after I decided to stay, I took a o‘ : course from Steve Weiland. Although other people had believed in my abilities, Steve joined P. David in believing in the ideas I brought to our discussions. For the first time in my graduate career, I began to believe that what I had to say was important. It sounds cliche these days, but Steve challenged me to be different. He taught me the rules and then showed me how to break them so I could what I wanted to say in the way I needed to say it. I am not sure how Steve and P. David had the perseverance to help a ‘techie’ become an academic. However, I am indebted to the support they provided. Their encouragement and advice helped secure both Spencer Research Training Grants I received to fund this research. I also wish to thank the other two members of my committee—Susan Florio-Ruane and Rand Spiro. Once P. David and Steve helped me set the course, Susan and Rand provided the scaffolding I needed to learn more about narrative and technology. Their feedback was priceless. Although I did not answer all the questions they asked along the way, they provided a lifetime of learning in a short amount of time. I need to thank the newest of my academic and personal mentors, Susan Melnick. She nurtured my growth as I made the transition from graduate student viii to outreach specialist. Her concern for my well being provided the space in which to complete this dissertation. My faith, family, and mentors provided the impetus to propose, write, defend, and revise this dissertation. However, without Danielle and Elizabeth, it would have never come to fruition. Danielle was willing to let me scrutinize her practice every single day for three years, including the final year of dissertation research. Danielle defined what a teacher was, how a good teacher taught, and what it meant to keep the faith. Elizabeth shared everything she could by telling her story. She took a risk in letting people see the ‘real’ Elizabeth, and I could not have asked for more. In sum, I hope and pray that the people who supported me see a part of themselves in this text. For truly, they have touched my life and I am grateful to them for their gifts. ix TABLE OF CONTENTS INTRODUCTION .................................................................................................. 1 WHAT Do WE KNow ABOUT TECHNOLOGY AND EDUCATION? .................................... 1 A MULTI-LAYERED APPROACH TO EDUCATIONAL TECHNOLOGY .................................. 4 A DEEPER PSYCHOLOGY OF TECHNOLOGY ................................................................... 7 SARAH’S STORY' ........................................................................................................... 10 ASSESSING SARAH’S STORY ........................................................................................ 13 RE-ASSESSING SARAH’S STORY .................................................................................. 15 A MORE COMPLETE ANALYSIS OF SARAH AND HER EMATE ....................................... 17 UNDERSTANDING WHAT MAKES A ‘GOOD INNOVATION' .............................................. 18 CHOOSING A CASE STUDY TO EXAMINE ‘GOOD INNOVATION’ IMPLEMENTATION ....... 21 GETTING To KNOW A PERSON ....................................................................................... 23 UNDERSTANDING THE SOCIAL CONTEXT ....................................................................... 25 “GOT MILK ?” ...................................................................................................... 28 CHAPTER 1: THE PEDAGOGY BEHIND “GOT MILK?” ................................... 35 INTRODUCTION .............................................................................................................. 35 AUTHENTIC, INTERESTING, AND CHALLENGING CONTENT .......................................... 38 A SENSE OF OWNERSHIP ............................................................................................. 44 ACTIVE PARTICIPATION, COLLABORATION, AND SOCIAL INTERACTION ....................... 45 THE CREATION OF ARTIFACTS ...................................................................................... 51 PUBLICATION, REFLECTION, AND FEEDBACK ............................................................... 53 A DISCUSSION OF “GOOD PEDAGOGY’ ......................................................................... 55 GOOD PEDAGOGY AND MORE ...................................................................................... 59 CHAPTER 2: THE PEOPLE BEHIND “GOT MILK?” ......................................... 61 INTRODUCTION .............................................................................................................. 61 WHO ARE THE MEMBERS OF AN INNOVATION? ............................................................. 63 THE PEOPLE BEHIND “GOT MILK?” .............................................................................. 64 THE DIALOGIC NATURE OF IMPLEMENTATION .............................................................. 65 GENUINE INTERACTION ................................................................................................. 68 THE FLEXIBLE NATURE OF TEACHING AND TECHNOLOGY ........................................... 71 OPPORTUNITIES FOR LEGITIMATE PARTICIPATION ........................................................ 76 A DISCUSSION OF GOOD PEOPLE ................................................................................ 78 MORE THAN JUST “GOOD PEOPLE” AND “GOOD PEDAGOGY” ..................................... 79 CHAPTER 3: THE PERFORMANCE OF “GOT MILK?” .................................... 82 INTRODUCTION .............................................................................................................. 82 APPROPRIATE USE OF TECHNOLOGIES ........................................................................ 85 COGNITIVE TOOLS To ASSESS LEARNING OUTCOMES ................................................ 87 DIVERSE METHODS FOR DEEP AND MORE COMPLETE ANALYSES ............................. 90 A DISCUSSION OF GOOD PERFORMANCE .................................................................... 93 CHAPTER 4: ELIZABETH’S STORY ................................................................. 96 CHAPTER 5: THE IMPACT OF “GOT MILK?” IMPLICATIONS FOR A DEEPER PSYCHOLOGY OF TECHNOLOGY ........... 103 THE “GOT MILK?” PROJECT AND THE THREE P’S ...................................................... 104 IMPLICATIONS OF ELIZABETH’S STORY AND THE THREE “P’s” ................................... 106 SUMMARY AND A CALL FOR FUTURE RESEARCH ...................................................... 114 APPENDIX A: INTERVIEW PROTOCOL ........................................................ 116 APPENDIX B: ELIZABETH’S PHILOSOPHY ON TEACHING WITH TECHNOLOGY ................................................................................................. 1 17 APPENDIX C: ELIZABETH’S STORY ............................................................. 120 xi Introduction What Do We Know About Technology_and Education? The arrival of personal computers and, more recently, the connection of those computers to the vast resources of the Internet, offer the potential to dramatically change the educational landscape.” (Microsoft, 1996, 15) Media do not influence Ieaming under any conditions. (Clark, 1983, p. 445) With the amount of research and research journals available on the subject, it may seem like we have established many ‘facts’ and ‘truths’ about technology and pedagogy. We have evidence that technology helps motivate certain children, particularly those with special needs (Bamberger, 1999; Englert, Zhao, & Ferdig, 1998). Technology provides students access to places and information they may not have had access to before (Hall, 1999). Students more productively navigate complex, ill-structured domains when they use tools such as hypermedia and multimedia (Spiro, Coulson, Feltovich, & Anderson, 1988). Add the available research to the enormous amount of time and money designated each year for technology implementation, and an outsider might be convinced we have gained many ‘facts’ and ‘truths’ about the role of technology in education. More realistically, the ‘truth’ is that we are only just beginning to learn within and about the young field of educational technology (understanding the adoption, use, and impact of technologies such aS computers in education). AS can be expected, we have ten questions for every one answer. Why do certain teachers adopt technology quickly while others refuse to implement it? Should every school have a computer lab or a laptop available for each student? Do media such as computers directly impact a students’ educational development or is it merely the instructional design afforded by the medium? How much lntemet access Should students be given? Do technologies fundamentally change a teacher’s practice or merely make it more efficient? All of these questions can potentially be encapsulated by (and are superimposed within) the caricatured battle currently waged in the field of educational technology. One side of the divide consists of technology-driven educators soap-boxing the classroom-Changing benefits of computer implementation. The other side is made up of technology critics voicing the concerns and warnings of yet another unjustified panacea for education. Although this dividing line is obviously more blurred and the differences more complex than portrayed in this story, the ideologies undergirding each Side are very real in the academic literature and the policy decisions in our schools (Agostino, 1999; Berrien, 1998; Cuban, 1986; Oppenheimer, 1997; Reeves, 1999). Technology‘, whether seen simply as devices with a central processing unit (CPU) or most recently as the lntemet and multimedia, is seemingly under scrutiny for some inherent abilities to help teachers teach, help learners learn, and to fundamentally Change the social and educational context of Classrooms. Rather than taking sides in this debate (labeled a futile approach by Thomas Reeves in a keynote address at EdMedia ’99 (Reeves, 1999), we return ‘ The dictionary (Webster, 2000) defines technology in numerous ways. For purposes of this dissertation, the word ‘technology’ will be used to refer to computers and their use in education. to our original question—what do we really know about technology and education? I will argue in this dissertation that to understand the role of technology in education is to recognize that technology and pedagogy are not and cannot be separated in educational technology. Thus, the advancements made through educational technology research have afforded convincing evidence because they have addressed technology as a part of pedagogy.2'3 Rather than defending or depending upon the intrinsic qualities of technologies, they have acknowledged the fact that technology (for education) is neither inherently good nor bad. Or, to paraphrase Norman (Norman, 1993): the good news is that technology can make us smart...the bad news is that technology can make uS stupid. Admittedly, there are qualities of different types of technologies that can make them more or less useful for certain types of activities—a point that will be elaborated upon in this text. For instance, a smaller computer that Children can hold and carry will allow for different types of interactions than say a desktop computer with a 21 ” monitor. Or, a desktop application that only allows five users at a time might limit the functionality in a high school computer lab. However, the point is that what makes a technology innovation good or bad does not solely reside in the technology itself. Rather, 2 Readers might be convinced that I am referring to local transfer. In other words, what we know about technology and education is only valid when it relates to a certain pedagogical instance. The argument would be that an innovation is good because the research describes technology implementation in a fourth- grade classroom (in Holland, Michigan) studying Shakespeare. Furthermore, it is useful because it can be transferred to other Holland fourth-grade classrooms that want to study Shakespeare. However, I am not referring to transfer between cases. I am describing educational technology research that focuses on technology in pedagogy rather than technology in and of itself. 3 Not only has research addressed technology as a part of pedagogy, but it has also addressed it as part of a cultural system (which includes pedagogy). Nicolopoulou and Cole (1994), through the Fifth Dimension project, argue that the effectiveness of programs will depend on the integration of those programs within a framework of the institution in which it is introduced, not on the intrinsic qualities of the technology in isolation. as I will argue in this dissertation, it is a complex process involving the technology, the personnel (innovators, educators, and learners), the pedagogy, and the relationships between the three. Although variations of the ideas in this text are evident in the existing, relevant literature, this is an important point to clarify and reiterate for a young field such as educational technology. Educators, technologists, and psychologists have decried the lack of research to justify the onslaught of technology expansion in our schools. They argue that there is no firm evidence that media even impact learning, as evidenced by the history of instructional strategies such as radio and television (Cuban, 1986). These criticisms notwithstanding, the field of educational technology has made important advancements regarding the qualities of successful technology integrations and the support they need for implementation. Thus, along with the call for further research to validate the technologies and defend their high cost, we need to revisit what we already know and what we have yet to Ieam. In other words, as our young domain grows we must ground development of cognitive tools in what we know currently works. A Multi-Layered Approach to Educational Technology Understanding ‘best practices’ in working with technology and education through an exploration of the relationships between technology, personnel and pedagogy will help ground past and present research, as well as guide the field into the future with focused research rather than numbing debates. However, ensuring more structured and rigid examinations of technology use is not enough. We need to ensure that the expansion of teChnology research encompasses the right kinds of questions—questions that incorporate assessments of the technology development, questions that address the types of human interaction that are necessary for implementations to attain predefined and unintentional goals, and questions that delve into the psychological nature of the relationships we have with technology.4 This is an important step as there is danger in the push that has been made for increased research on technology and its impact on Ieaming. Specifically, educational technology research programs tend to focus solely on the cognitive domain in the relationship between technology and Ieaming. This is not to suggest that measuring Ieaming outcomes of technology use is an unnecessary or fruitless endeavor, only one with inevitable limits. There is a plain, one might say urgent, need for a more complete approach to research on technology integration. The problem with the trend in educational technology (often situated in educational psychology programs) to focus solely on the cognitive domain is the disappearance of research addressing the relationship between technology and social and emotional development. Surveys of major refereed technology journals indicate an almost extinct population of articles relating to this affective domain (see Jones & Paolucci, 1998, for a complete review). A number of articles have attempted to break out of the cognitive domain by asking questions about perceptions, attitudes, and motivation in using technology (Cordova & 4 Mark Windschitl also makes this claim, arguing for stronger research that poses more critical questions (Windschitl, 1998). Lepper, 1996; Lepper & Hodell, 1989). Others have begun to explore the relationships that exist because of or with technologies such as computers. Sherry Turkle, for instance, provided evidence that some people actually adapt their personalities to the personas they develop and adopt in on-Iine communities (T urkle, 1995). Byron Reeves and Clifford Nass have asked us to rethink media such as computers as social actors in a media equation (media = life) (Reeves & Nass, 1996). However, as highlighted by the aforementioned surveys on educational technology research, these types of studies are becoming more and more rare.5 Studies like these are leading the way for creating a new set of research tools, but we must continue to focus on gaining a more complete psychology of technology. Clifford Geertz argues that we need “thick descriptions” in order to better represent and understand human experience (Geertz, 1983). And in his famous essay “Deep Play” (Geertz, 1973b), he supplies a timely metaphor for a new field like educational technology. Applying this metaphor of “depth” to educational technology essentially means opening it up to all of the tools available to us as educators and psychologists rather than just technologists. There are a number of psychological questions that, although normally associated with developmental psychology and psychoanalysis, may prove fruitful in our discussion of a “deeper" educational technology. Questions include the role of emotions through technology, mediating relationships with technology, and sense and meaning- ’ Jones and Paolucci (see (Jones & Paolucci, 1998) surveyed eight major refereed journals over a three-year time period. Most of the articles in the journal publications addressed technology applications, development, or implementation. Only about 18% of all technology research completed addressed an evaluation of learning outcomes. However, statistics indicate the almost non-existence of articles relating to the affective domain. making through technology. Many of these questions fall outside of the realm of the cognitive domain. However, answering these questions will not only provide resources to satisfy cognitive concerns, but it will also provide a more complete representation of experiences in technology integration. We need to work on broadening this relatively new field. We do not have the accounts we need of the emotional and social development of students to new educational technologies. The task of technology-focused, educational psychologists and teachers, then, is not only to establish more structured research and teaching agendas, but also to expand the diversity within those inquiries. I will also argue in this dissertation that obtaining a deeper psychology of technology will afiord a more multi-layered and complete understanding of pedagogy and technology implementation. A Deeper Psychology of Technology A driving question for this research revolves around an exact definition of deeper (or “depth”) in trying to obtain a deeper psychology of technology. Webster (Webster, 2000) defines deep as: 1: extending far from some surface or area; a: extending far downward; b: extending inward from an outer surface 2: characterized by profundity of feeling or quality A deeper psychology of technology might therefore imply a profound examination of the uses of technology as defined by psychological terms. The problem with a Simple definition like this is the relationship of deep psychology to psychoanalytic thinking. Somewhere in the term “deep psychology” is the idea that if we dug far enough into the psyche of the individual using technology, we would discover beliefs and motives that justified the behaviors of the subject. Coming to know a person who is using the technology, rather than focusing solely on the use of the technology itself, may be labeled underthe banner of “people-centered analyses of technology use.” The goal would be to examine the person who is using the technology and the ensuing relationship with the technology, rather than assuming that the characteristics and uses of the technology are somehow inherent in the technology itself. Mass and Reeves’ work (Reeves & Nass, 1996), which concentrates on what they term the “media equation’, offers evidence that humans enter into social contracts and relationships with technology. They argue that interactions with new media like television and computers are fundamentally social in nature. Much like interactions in real life, people expect media to obey a wide range of social and natural rules. Thus, coming to a more complete understanding of the individual and the human intentions they confer upon the technology is an important step in understanding effects of technology use. However, a deeper psychology of technology goes beyond merely addressing the human and his/her relationship with a technology. It is more concerned with expanding the vocabulary available to technologists and researchers as they explore educational investigations with technologies such as computers. This deeper approach implies not a psychoanalytic View of the individual, but rather a complete, comprehensive, and inclusive description of the instance of technology use. This research might entail examining the constructions a person is able to make through and with the technology about the learning objective. Conversely, it might also address what knowledge an individual constructs about the technology itself, and the resulting differences and Similarities with the constructions made by the teacher or the developer. More broadly Speaking, research might also focus on the interactions that surround or are enhanced and scaffolded through the use of the technology. This would include the discourse between participants using a technology, the dialogue sustained through the technology, or an examination of the activity as a whole to determine what learning opportunities were supported and neglected while interacting with the technology. This is not to argue that previous research has not attempted to meet any or all of these objectives. Rather, it is to suggest that many of these studies have stopped short of providing a comprehensive description of the events surrounding the technology use. Many instances of research have ended when achievements were assessed through behavioral measures such as test scores. Again, this type of research is not a wasted effort. Our growing field is in need of research that highlights cognitive gains made through educational technology. These types of studies have helped shed light on the technology effectiveness debate mentioned earlier. The fear, however, is that an incomplete (or less than comprehensive) description of the events lends itself to incomplete generalizations about future technology use. More complete descriptions and analyses of instances of computer use will foster better opportunities to label interactions with technology in general. Educational Psychology, the discipline in which most educational technology programs are situated, offers itself as a prime source of extending and expanding a complete vocabulary for our new field. Psychology can provide the vocabulary that as descriptive and prescriptive tools to educational researchers and technologists. In sum, those who study educational technology are determined to understand broadly why certain technologies are effective, and Specifically why individuals interact the way they do with various technologies. This dissertation addresses the same objectives while attempting deeper and more complete analyses of innovation implementation. Sarah’s Stopr‘s'7 The struggle to understand technology innovations in education became vital for me in the summer of 1997. l was working with a team of graduate students and faculty at Michigan State University in a project aimed at implementing technology into special education and inclusion Classrooms at a local elementary school. As part of the “TELE—Web” (Technology Enhanced Learning Environment-Webs) project, the team decided to purchase an eMate for each of the three classrooms in the study. (An eMate is an Apple product, a grandchild of the popular Newton series.) There were two main reasons for the purchase. First, special education children sometimes have a hard time typing 6 Sarah’s story was first told at the Society of Information technology and Teacher Education (SITE) Annual Conference in San Antonio, Texas (1999), where it won “Best Research Paper” (Ferdig & Weiland, 1999) 7 An experience much like mine with Sarah, Jeanne Bamberger tells a very fascinating story about ‘Leon’—a quiet student who had extraordinary insights when giving the pace and conceptual space he needed to make new knowledge (see (Bamberger, 1999). 3 TELE-Web is funded by the US. Department of Education and Michigan State University’s College of Education. http://tele.educ.msu.edu/ 10 on the computer. The researchers and teachers felt that if students had an opportunity to write on the screen, a technology the eMate provides, they may be more motivated to practice spelling, write stories, and read works to their colleagues and teacher. Second, the eMate is fun to use. The research team thought it would be interesting to see how the Children reacted to new and exciting technologies in the classroom. Shortly after the eMates were introduced, I began to work with one of the fourth grade students named Sarah.9 Sarah, labeled by the school psychologist as “Educable Mentally Impaired (EMI)”, had bonded with the eMate. She used it whenever she was required to complete a spelling test on a computer (she also had the choice of one of the “bigger“ computers). She also used it to write stories whenever she had free time. From a cognitive perspective, Sarah made tremendous improvements over the course of the year. Her standardized reading test scores (SORT & Durrell‘o) as well as her general classroom grades improved. However, there was more to Sarah’s story than mere test score improvements. Sarah and her computer “Brian” became friends. The bonding could best be described as an intense, human-like experience. Not only did she talk to the computer (and assure the team members that it talked back), but she also named him and took care of daily needs such as its feeding. On occasion, She was even seen taking it to the bathroom. 9 Pseudonyms have been used for all school and participant names in this dissertation to protect identities. '0 The Durrell Analysis of Reading Difficulty and the Slosson Oral Reading Test (SORT) are two standardized literacy tests given to measure reading level and word recognition (respectively). 11 At first, the teacher and the team member considered the situation nothing more than a curious phenomenon. However, Sarah’s relationship with the eMate became more striking when she began using it to mediate relationships both in and out of school. In the classroom, she was one of the only girls in the resource room. She did not get along with many of the boys and complained often about the lack of girls in the class. She even wrote the following “complaint” and addressed it to Brian (translated for meaning): I sometimes want a girl but I can't get a girl. Mrs. K. will not let me get one. All my life I prayed for a girl but you are the only one I can talk to. However, She decided to have her computer “be a boy.” “Sarah needs to control things” her teacher commented. “This may be her way of controlling the situation with the boys in the class.” When asked about how she knew the eMate was male, She responded with “cause I work on boys all of the time. I'm surrounded by them." Sarah also mediated her relationship with her teacher via the eMate. Mrs. K would often help students evaluate their stories. The eMate took over this role and would tell Sarah which of her stories were good, which ones needed revisions, and which ones should be deleted. Mrs. K explained that for Sarah, the eMate represented manageable production. She would often say, “Look what I’ve done, Mrs. K.” Mrs. K commented, “She is proud of what she has done with the eMate." School was not the only environment in which Sarah mediated with the eMate. At a party during the school year, her aunt was murdered. She told us 12 that She could not tell Brian about the death because “it would cry.” She Spent much of the next few weeks after the death without Brian. It took her quite a while to learn how to tell “it” what had happened. Assessing Sarah’s Stopy How does one Ieam to make sense of Sarah’s story? Sarah was a poor student going through difficult times. However, she was not only able to maintain her composure, She was able to improve herself as a student and as a person (her teachers and her mother noted her improved behavior; Sarah, herself, commented on her increased happiness and satisfaction with school and ‘life’). Did (and if so how) the relationship between the eMate and Sarah facilitate this growth? It would be very easy to believe in the power of technology as you hear about a very emotionally troubled, at-risk student grow into a classroom representative, school leader, and better-than-average student through an interaction with a computer. It would be very easy to sympathize with the numerous teachers who heard the story and wanted an eMate for every student in their Classes. It would even be fair to join with other researchers in the call for more exploration of eMate use in the classroom. However, as educators interested in technology research, the most important lesson to Ieam from Sarah’s case is how important it is for us is to be able to explain (as best as possible) instances of computer use such as Sarah’s story, to understand how and why this technology played such a powerful role in her life. 13 Notice first the pedagogy and instructional design afforded by the design of the technology. The size and battery power of the eMate allowed the computer to be in front of Sarah longer than the time spent in front of the larger, desktop computers or the one, heavy school laptop. The weight, Size, and shape allowed her to hold, control, even hug a very interactive tool. She was able to take it under the table (her secret writing room), drag it with her to the playground (literally), and feed it during her lunch hour. The advantages of the design, in turn, provided more writing time during the school day, regardless of where Sarah was in the classroom (or out of the classroom). The teacher’s main objective of increased writing time was certainly provided by Sarah’s use of the eMate. Just as important as the pedagogy was the personnel involved in the instruction. It is true that without the eMate, the instructor may not have been able to provide as much writing time for Sarah (although, as I am sure Richard Clark11 would argue, we could probably find other media to accomplish the same important goal). However, without a competent instructor who knew how to use the eMate (both technologically and pedagogically), the tool may not have been used, or may have not been used to help students like Sarah. The classroom teacher knew enough technologically about the eMate to be able to get Sarah started (how to turn it on, how to create a new file, etc.). Moreover, she knew " In a very famous article, Richard Clark makes the argument that technology influences Ieaming as much a truck delivering groceries influences our nutrition (Clark, 1983); also see (Clark, 1994). The argument extends to the claim that media can replace each other. In this case, the eMate afforded something, but the al'gllment would claim that other media could potentially do the same. Learn more about the ‘Great Media Debate’ between Richard Clark and Robert Kozma online at: httPz//hagar.up.ac.za/rbo/construct/media.htmI l4 enough about what it could do to successfully create instruction for Sarah and her ‘friend’. Finally, she knew enough about Sarah to know what instruction with the eMate Sarah could handle by herself. Through the use of a technology, a strong writing curriculum, and a knowledgeable adult, Sarah completed group and individually assigned projects. She also spent a lot of her own time (recess, lunch, etc.) working on the computer. At the end of the project, the main goal of helping her develop literacy skills was realized. Sarah had learned to type, write stories, and Spell better. Her classroom grades, especially those related to literacy, improved. Furthermore, her test scores on the SORT and Durrell standardized tests increased significantly (p<.05). Re-assessing Sarah’s Stopr The initial research questions focused on cognitive growth as evidenced by that development of literacy skills (measured by standardized test scores). The accompanying research findings suggested that the eMate might make a promising tool for developing and improving literacy Skills. Those questions and findings, however, had nothing to offer the research or the researchers when presented with a young girl initiating a human-like bond with a computer. Initially, some of the team members wondered whether Sarah’s interaction with the eMate was similar to a Child having an imaginary friend or a doll. The eMate was a safe tool for Sarah. All of the students knew the importance that the teacher and visitors from the university had placed on technology. Her classmates, peers, and instructors would grant her more agency if she bonded 15 with the eMate than they would have if She had bonded with another toy, personal belonging (blanket) or instructional artifact. But, how did this differ from the actual psychological use of a blanket, doll, or imaginary friend? Trying to further understand these happenings, we turned to the psychological explanations of the influential British developmentalist D.W. Winnicott (1896- 1971). In his widely Cited "Playing & Reality", Winnicott (Winnicott, 1971) suggests that infants, children, and even adults make use of “transitional objects.” Transitional objects are objects or phenomena that are related both to external and internal reality. This intermediate area of experience, unchallenged in respect of its belonging to inner or external (shared) reality, constitutes the greater part of the infant's experience, and throughout life is retained in the intense experience that belongs to the arts and to religion and to imaginative living, and to creative scientific work. (Winnicott, 1971, p.242). We hypothesized that Sarah was using the eMate as a transitional object between her internal reality and the reality that existed in the Classroom and at home. In other words, it is probable that She was using the eMate much like a blanket, doll, or imaginary friend. Viewing the eMate as merely a computer used for spelling tests, even if it was important in her obtaining higher reading scores, one would have missed this “thicker description” of what was really happening and thus a more probing psychological account.12 '2 This application from Winnicott is but a small sign of the potential uses of psychological ideas in the study of Sarah and of educational technology in general. Other interpretations might have been suggested from areas such as Cultural Psychology (Cole, 1996) or Narrative Psychology (Bruner, 1996). 16 A More Complete Ana_lvsis of Sarah and her eMate Sarah’s story of technology use is inspiring. However, it would be faulty to conclude that teachers should use eMates in the classroom just because one student Showed improved literacy test scores after using it. As a matter of fact, I believe it would be errant to make the Claim even if one hundred students showed improved marks, without first coming to a more complete analysis of the Situation. Realizing that comprehensive analysis, at least in this case, meant understanding both the psychology and pedagogy behind the tool use as well as the individual(s) who acted as the more knowledgeable or more capable others for Sarah (in Sarah’s case, her teacher, and on occasion her parents, her friends, and even the eMate). Sarah was not the only learner in this story. The teacher and Sarah’s parents were able to better plan future educational activities for Sarah at school and at home. The teacher educators in the project learned more about what kind of knowledge a teacher needed to effectively use a technology. And the project researchers could design future research agendas around aforementioned evidence (i.e., the eMate was used as part of the writing curriculum). In sum, through an understanding of the pedagogy and personnel behind the tool, both the teacher and the research team were better able to understand, (more importantly) explain, and potentially replicate the cognitive gains Sarah had made. However, stopping at the point of reinforcing or better understanding cognitive gains may have meant missing a more complete picture of the study. 17 Coming to terms with how Sarah was using the eMate also involved developing a deep understanding of the psychology behind the relationship she initiated. Attaining that depth helped the teachers and researchers better comprehend her emotional development and how to foster her emotional, intellectual, and social growth in the Classroom. As the teacher said, “I now better understand how to reach Sarah.“ Drawing on the experiences in the social life of the classroom, the teacher was better able to help the parent understand the ways in which Sarah’s behavior was changing at home. Sarah’s mom, at a parent-teacher conference, told the teacher that Sarah was more willing to share thoughts and feelings and participated more in family activities. Finally, it opened up new possibilities for the use of the technology with other social and emotionally disturbed children. A deep psychological exploration helped teachers and researchers realize that the development of questions and the analyses of the innovation did not start and end with cognitive gains (or the lack thereof). Rather, it grounded the experience of the innovation in the personnel, pedagogy, and psychology that made up the innovation. Understanding What Makes a ‘Good Innovation’ In describing a technology used to improve writing skills, Krendl et al. (Krendl, 1996) claim that a rich technology experience is not defined by any one feature, but by a combination of elements (the child, the adult, the technology, the setting, etc.). Therefore, as echoed by this dissertation, the research mandate for educational technology. and one way to achieve a deeper 18 psychology of technology, is to explore the ways in which these different elements interact to create a ‘good innovation.’13 What exactly do we mean by a ‘good’ innovation? Was the eMate in Sarah’s case an example of a good innovation? Many would define ‘good innovation’ as one that improves performance, successfully meets a pre—defined plan or solves an educational problem (in this case, through the use of technology such as computers). For instance, a good innovation would use technology to help students become better readers. Or, a good innovation would increase SAT scores after continued computer use. I argue in this dissertation that there are at least three necessary elements that characterize a good technology innovation. Those three elements both define and answer the question of what makes a good innovation. A good technology innovation requires the “Three P’s”: 1) Good Pedagogy 2) Good People 3) Good Performance The first two, the good pedagogy and the good people, help define the term ‘good innovation’. A good innovation is one where technology and pedagogy are not separated. A good innovation engages a process that enhances the relationships among innovator, educator, and Ieamer. Unfortunately, in much of the available research, pedagogy and personnel take a backseat to an interest in cognitive gains. Thus, asking whether an innovation '3 The mandate for research to understand, explain, and describe what makes a good computer innovation has been reiterated numerous times in the literature. The first example that comes to mind is Sharon Derry & Susanne Lajoie, who state that the impetus for editing Computers as Cognitive Tools was to understand what good pedagogy is and how computers systems will enhance that instruction to create good pedagogical tools (Derry & Lajoie, 1993). A second instance is Thomas Reeves’ invited address at the 1999 Educational Media Conference (Reeves, I999). Reeves argues that we need to focus our energies on the invention, understanding, and improvement of creative learning technologies. 19 improves SAT scores or improves reading skills may obscure important factors that will later prove important in helping understand, explain, or replicate the research findings. The third factor, Good Performance, helps answer this question: if a good innovation is one in which the pedagogy and the technology are Closely linked, how can one tell if the performance (that is, the outcomes and the implementation) of the innovation was good, successful, etc.? At this level, we can draw from psychology to afford a more multi-Iayered and complete understanding of the performance of the technology and its implementation into pedagogy. This third level may indeed use cognitive psychology to assess changes in SAT scores or improved reading levels. However, it may also explore the innovation as something that helps a child make a transition, mediate with others, or learn more about herself. The claim is that good is not defined solely on cognitive measures. Given this definition, the use of the eMate with Sarah was a good technology innovation on two levels. First, the innovation was pedagogically sound; it was well designed, rooted in a strong writing curriculum, and supervised by a more knowledgeable other. Second, the eMate produced good results both cognitively and affectively. In this sense, good refers to the success or failure of the goals of the technology innovation. Cognitive gains, the intended consequences of the use of the technology, were measured by standardized literacy tests. Affective gains, the unintended consequences of the technology use, were rendered plausible by using a framework (D.W. Winnicott’s theory of 20 transitional objects) to explain how and why it facilitated Sarah’s learning and development. The main goal of a deep, psychological examination of an innovation is to ensure a deep and complete enough analysis so that: a) cognitive changes, whether they exist or not, can be explained in relation to the person using the technology in a curriculum, rather than explaining it as an intrinsic quality of the technology itself; b) stronger and more definitive claims can be made about the instance of implementation; C) teacher educators are provided with more comprehensive information about what and how to train future teachers or in- service teachers learning technology; and d) unintended consequences, mostly appearing as affective, social, and emotional Changes, can be measured. Choosing a Case Study to Exa‘mine ‘Good lnnovation’ lm_p_le_meLati