. ...:..;..; v .3 "a“: «an; {'33 r 1 FM. .M LA :T 957 4. 2.3.7 ..(. _ 3: 3:7: . if w >:.~"?i M) ‘ V 5 1...: ,. 2} I: F. $5. 43$ 11v ‘ In (‘4. .Kac. . anti ‘3? 7L 5.. 1 $0.? 53%»..6‘“; THESIS / 4... __~’"’(‘( 1-( This is to certify that the thesis entitled Vowel Systems of African Englishes: and Perceptual Analysis Acoustic presented by Mungai Mutonya has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for Ph.D. Linguistics degree in , L/ , 0- Major professor Date June: is, 2000 MS U is an Affirmative Action/Equal Opportunity Institution LIBRARY Michigan State University PLACE lN REIURN BOX to remove this checkout from your record. To AVOID FINES return on or before date due. MAY BE RECALLED with earlier due date if requested. ‘ DATE DUE MAY 2 32002 | DATE DUE DATE DUE 4 ocrobzum Dawn i‘[‘-‘ i} 8 EOCS 1004 JAN 8 0629 06 11m C‘JCIRC/Datth-pas-pjd #_fl/ VOWEL SYSTEMS OF AFRICAN ENGLISHES: ACOUSTIC AND PERCEPTUAL ANALYSIS By Mungai Mutonya A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University In partial fulfillment of the requirements For the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Linguistics, Germanic, Slavic, Asian and African Languages 2000 ABSTRACT VOWELS SYSTEMS OF AFRICAN ENGLISHES: ACOUSTIC AND PERCEPTUAL ANALYSIS MUNGAIIIBVIIUTONYA This study analyzes production and perception of African English (AfrE) vowels by university students in Ghana, Kenya and Zimbabwe. Acoustic and perception tests are carried out and subsequently correlated in order to determine the characteristic of vowel systems in each sample. In order to determine the acoustic qualities of vowels, data was initially elicited from language groups with relatively homogeneous social and linguistic background. F1 and F2 scores of onset (steady state) vowels contained in eleven English monosyllabic were calculated and subsequently plotted using computer software; Signalyze (version 3.12) and Plotnik (version04) respectively. To account for vowel perception, data was elicited from AfrE listeners using the minimal pair and identification test methods. Both acoustic and perception results challenge previously held assumptions regarding vowel variance in west, east and southern Africa. Ghanaian respondents in this study did not back RP central vowels [3] and [A] to [D ], a position so strongly held in earlier studies. Moreover, vowel system identity among ‘Bantu English’ speakers in eastern and southern Africa is not corroborated by Kenyan and Zimbabwean respondents in this study. Gender distinction within and across varieties is also identified. Copyright by MUNGAI MUTONYA 2000 DEDICATION I dedicate this work to my wife Laurah, my sons Mwariri and Mutonya, our parents, brothers and sisters. Could not have made it without your love and support. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS I am greatly indebted to many people who made substantive contributions, directly or indirectly, to this research project. Their inspiration, support and guidance has influenced the completion of this study. Needless to say, there have been numerous tough hurdles to clear, many sacrifices to make, short and long term goals to meet, numerous responsibilities to bear, and of course, a dissertation to write. Such challenges would have been insurmountable without the valuable insights, guidance, love and support from my professors, colleagues, friends and family members. To them all, I extend my grateful acknowledgements. I have been lucky to work with a great teacher and sociolinguist, Dr. Dennis R. Preston, my mentor and advisor. From the formative stages of this study, through data collection, data analysis, to the completion of this research, Dennis has always been available to answer any questions, address any research concerns, and to read the drafts whenever I had one ready for him. 1 thanks him most sincerely. Other members of my dissertation committee have equally informed the study in numerous ways: Dr. David Dwyer has enriched the study through his expertise on African linguistics and through his very constructive suggestions; Dr. Yen-Hwei Lin provided valuable insights on phonology and language variation; Dr. Denise Troutman vi painstakingly read my draft and gave very thoughtful and valuable insights. I am equally grateful to Dr. Geneva Smitherrnan, who agreed to participate in my dissertation defense as an outside reader. Her comments and suggestions helped ameliorate this final draft. I thank them all for their wise guidance. Dr. Harry Akussa of University of Accra, Ghana; Dr. Patrick Kambewa of Malawi; Dr. Abraham Gitau Ndungu of Kenyatta University, Kenya; Maina Mutonya, currently a graduate student at the University of Witwatersrand, South Africa; and Mwangi Mutonya, assisted me with data collection. Without their assistance and the cooperation of all the respondents they interviewed, this research would not have taken its current form. I thank them most sincerely. The vicissitudes and sacrifices encountered through Graduate school could not have been easily borne without the incredible support I received from my beloved family. My greatest appreciation and gratitude goes to my loving wife Laurah for the love, patience, support and friendship through the years; my precious boys, Mwariri and Mutonya for giving me a cherished learning, and the motivation to accomplish this task; my loving parents Jeremiah Mutonya and Rebecca Njoki for their inspiration and a firm educational foundation; the Mutonya family - Njuguna, N joroge, Mari, Wangui, Wanjiru, Kamau, Muchiri, Maina, Mwangi, Mukami, Machira, and all the youngsters for their support, and for daring to dream. Thanks to my in-laws for the support, especially my late father—in-law, Jotham Mwariri Gichura, who supported and encouraged me. May his soul rest in peace. Thanks to Beatrice Wambui Cege for sharing her life with us. I thank Michigan State University and in particular the chair of Department of Linguistics, Dr. George Peters, for the Teaching Assistant Fellowship that financed my vii graduate study. Laurie Koehler was more than a Graduate Secretary to us; she was a friend, a dear friend with a warm smile, an attentive ear, and a loving heart that radiated warmth and sunshine to our department. I am grateful to Saisunnee Visanyoongoon and Li Qing, for sharing the trials and rigors of comprehensive exams. I am thankful to fellow sociolinguists who shared in the joy of Preston’s classes, the companionships in the language lab, and the inevitable sleepless nights during comps and dissertation writing. Thanks to Cege, Kuria, Smucker, Wangui, Walter, and Jamila for the solidarity, and to all my Kiswahili students for making it memorable. The faculty and staff of the African and Afro-American Studies program at Washington University in Saint Louis provided me with the necessary support and conducive environment that expedited the completion of my dissertation work. I am especially grateful to Professors Gerald Early, Rafia Zafar, Priscilla Stone, Tim Parsons and Richard Kisiara for their continued support. Adele Tuchler and Raye Riggins have made my life at Wash U and AFAS in particular more exciting. Thank you all. I am very grateful to the University of Nairobi, Kenya and the Department of Linguistics and African Languages for granting me study leave to pursue my doctoral studies at Michigan State University. I am grateful to Mzalendo Kibunja and Carolyn Harford for facilitating the opportunity. To all my teachers, classmates, friends and relatives who have positively impacted my educational pursuits, I say, ASANTE SANA. THAAAAIIII. viii TABLE OF CONTENTS List of Tables ..................................................................... xiii List of Figures ............................................................... xiv Chapter 1: Introduction 1. 0. An Overview of the Study ................................................... l l. 1 Statement of the Problem ................................................... . 3 1. 2 Background to the study ................................................... 5 1. 3 Theoretical Framework ................................................... 7 l. 4 Scope and Limitations ................................................... 11 1. 5 Data Collection ............................................... 13 l. 6. Data Analysis ................................................... ' 16 Chapter 2: Background to the Study 2.1. 0 Introduction .................................................................. 16 2. 2. 0 Background to the study ................................................... l6 2. 2. 1 African English? .......................................................... 16 2. 2. 2 AfrE’s Regional Vowel Systems .......................................... 19 2. 2. 2. 1 West African English (WAfrE) .......................................... 23 2. 2. 2. 2 East African English (EAfrE) .......................................... 29 ix 2. 2. 2. 3 Southern African English (SAfrE) ............................... 31 Chapter 32Theoretical Framework 3. l. 0 Introduction ..................................................................... 33 3. 2. 0 Theoretical Framework ...................................................... 33 3. 2.1 Some General Theoretical Assumptions .................................... 34 3. 3. 0 Theoretical Assumptions on Non-Native Speech Perception ........... 39 3. 4. 0 The Native Language Magnet Theory .................................... 42 3. 5. 0 Acoustic Theory of Speech Production ................................... 44 3.5. l Vowels ........................................................................ 45 3. 6. 0 Assumptions About Phonemic Identity ................................... 46 3. 6. I Introduction ................................................................... 46 3. 6. 2 On Defining a Phoneme: A Brief Historical Overview ............... 46 3. 6. 2. l Phonetic and Phonemic identity ......................................... 47 Chapter 4: Methodology 4. 1 Introduction .................................................................... 53 4. 2 Collection of Acoustic Data ................................................ 53 4. 2. l The Production- Sample ................................................... 53 4. 2. 2 Data Collection ............................................................. 54 4. 3. 0 Data Analysis ................................................................ 54 4. 3. 1 Acoustic Measurement ...................................................... 54 4. 4. 0 Vowel Perception Data ...................................................... 56 4. 4. 1 Data Collection Methods ................................................. 4. 4. 2 The Perception-Sample ...................................................... Chapter 5: Data Analysis 5. l. 0 Data Presentation and Discussion ........................................... 5. l. l Schmied's Analysis of AfrE Vowel Systems .............................. 5. 2. 0 An Empirical Analysis of AfrE vowel systems ........................... 5.2. l Kenyan speakers ............................................................... 5. 2 .2 Male respondents .............................................................. 5. 2. 3 Female Respondents .......................................................... 5. 3. O Zimbabwean Speakers ....................................................... 5. 3. 1 Male Respondents ............................................................ 5. 3. 2 Female Respondents ......................................................... 5. 4. 0 Ghanaian Speakers ............................................................ 5. 4. 1 Male Respondents ............................................................ 5. 4. 2 Female Respondents .................................................... ’ ..... 5. 5. O A Comparative Analysis of Acoustic Results ........................... 5. 6. 1 Kenyans ..................................................................... 5. 6. 2 Ghanaians .................................................................... 5. 6. 3 Zimbabweans ................................................................ 5. 7. O AfrE Listeners’ Vowel Perception .......................................... 5.7. 1 Data Analysis .................................................................. 5. 8. 0 Identification Results and Discussion ...................................... xi 56 57 59 6O 64 64 66 69 72 74 76 80 8] 83 86 88 88 89 9O 9O 92 5. 9. 0 Results of the MPT ............................................................ 108 Chapter 6: Conclusions 6. 1 Introduction .................................................................... 112 6. 2 Correlating Acoustic and Perceptual Results ................................. l 13 6. 3 Theoretical Assumptions Revisited ............................................ l 18 Appendix A’ ............................................................................. 125 Appendix A ............................................................................. 126 Appendix B ............................................................................. 129 Appendix C ............................................................................. 141 Appendix D ............................................................................. 143 Appendix E ............................................................................. 148 Appendix F ............................................................................. 156 References ............................................................................. 159 xii LIST OF TABLES Table 1 — Identification Results of Vowel [i] .............................. 91 xiii LIST OF FIGURES Figure 1 - Adapted Representation of Schmied's Classic Comparison of RP, WAfrE, and EAfrE ........................................................... Figure 2 — WAfrE Vowel System as Presented by Trudgill and Hannah ....... Figure 3 - Todd’s Representation of the Vowel System of Cameroon English Figure 4 — Sey’s Comparison of RP and Ghanaian Vowel Systems ............ Figure 5 — A Comparison of RP, Nigerian and Cameroonian Vowel systems Figure 6 — The Realization of RP Schwa in Nigerian and Cameroonian Englishes Figure 7 — A Representation of the RP Vowel system as Illustrated in Figure 1 Figure 8 — A Representation of WAfrE as Illustrated in Figure 1 ................. Figure 9 — A Representation of EAfrE as Illustrated in Figure l .................. Figure 10 — Vowel Plot ofAll Kenyan Respondents........................ ....... Figure 10.1 - Njenga’s Vowel System ( a Kenyan Male) ....................... Figure 10.2 - Njogu’s Vowel system ( a Kenyan Male) ........................... Figure 10.3 - Mukami’s Vowel System ( a Kenyan female) ...................... Figure 10.4 - Kamba’s Vowel System ( a Kenyan Female) ....................... Figure 11 - Vowel Plot of All Zimbabwean Respondents ........................ Figure 1 1.1 - Jones’ Vowel System (a Zimbabwean Male) ....................... Figure 11.2 - Ndanga’s Vowel System (a Zimbabwean Male) .................. xiv 26 28 29 62 63 65 67 68 70 71 73 74 76 Figure 11.3 - Kapinda’s Vowel System ( a Zimbabwean Female) ............... 77 Figure 11.4 - Tamika’s Vowel System ( a Zimbabwean Female) ................. 78 Figure 12 - Vowel Plot of All Ghanaian Respondents .............................. 80 Figure 12.1- Kwame’s Vowel System ( a Ghanaian Male) ........................ 81 Figure 12.2 - Kojo’s Vowel System ( a Ghanaian Male) .......................... 82 Figure 12.3 - Akosua’s Vowel System ( a Ghanaian Female) ..................... 84 Figure 12.4 - Efua’s Vowel System ( a Ghanaian Female) ......................... 85 Figure 13.1— AfrE Listeners’ Identification of [i], [u], [e] ........................... 93 Figure 13.2- AfrE Listeners’ Identification of [i], [u], [e] ........................... 94 Figure 14— AfrE Listeners’ Identification of [ie], [0i][ua], [ju] ..................... 94 Figure 15.1- AfrE Listeners’ Identification of [a] .................................... 95 Figure 15.2- AfrE Listeners’ Identification of [a] ................................... 96 Figure 16.1- AfrE Listeners’ Identification Of[A] ........ 97 Figure 16.2- AfrE Listeners’ Identification of [A] .................................... 97 Figure 17.1- AfrE Listeners’ Identification of [3] ................................... 99 Figure 17.2- AfrE Listeners’ Identification of [3] ................................... 99 Figure 18.1- AfrE Listeners’ Identification of [I] ................................... 101 Figure 18.2- AfrE Listeners’ Identification of [I] ................................... 101 Figure 19.1- AfrE Listeners’ Identification of [D] ................................... 102 XV Figure 19.2 - AfrE Listeners’ Identification of [D] ................................. 103 Figure 20.1- AfrE Listeners’ Identification of [e] .................................... 104 Figure 20.2- AfrE Listeners’ Identification of [8] ..................................... 105 Figure 21- AfrE Listeners’ Identification of [ai] ..................................... 105 Figure 22 - AfrE Listeners’ Identification of [au] ..................................... 106 Figure 23- AfrE Listeners’ Identification of [ou] ...................................... 106 Figure 24 — Ghanaians Discrimination of Minimal Pairs .............................. 109 Figure 25 — Kenyans Discrimination of Minimal Pairs ................................. 110 Figure 26 — Zimbabweans Discrimination of Minimal Pairs ........................... 111 xvi Chapter One: Introduction Introduction In an earlier study, Mutonya (1997) observed that university students in three African countries positively identified regional varieties of African Engish (AfrE), and they assigned contrasting attitudinal values towards local and non-local varieties based on degree of accentedness. The scope of the study, however, did not allow for the measurement of physical and nonphysical qualities of the sounds that respondents reacted to in their judgments. Consequently, the current study focuses on the measurement of acoustic and perceptual characteristics of AfrE vowel systems. In general, the current study further investigates the nature of documented varieties of AfrE; West African English (WAfrE), East African English (EAfrE), and Southern African English (SAfrE). Specifically, the study seeks to determine what the vowel system of each regional variety looks like based on acoustic and perceptual analysis of data elicited from university students from Ghana, Kenya and Zimbabwe. However, unlike previous research in this field, the current study has paid considerable attention to empirical methods of sampling, data collection, acoustic and perceptual analysis, in an effort to tease out the most probable vowel systems in each region. 1. 0 An Overview of the Study The present study investigates the production and perception of English (L2) vowels by university students in three African countries. While holding L1 dialectal factors as homogeneous as possible, F1 and F2 scores of African English (AfrE) vowels produced by multilingual speakers from Ghana, Zimbabwe and Kenya were measured and analyzed. Furthermore, AfrE listeners' perception of Received Pronunciation (RP) vowels were analyzed and subsequently correlated with acoustic results. The probable vowel system for each group was determined by correlating the more physical attributes of the vowels (production) with the more abstract ones (perception). Consequently, conclusions drawn in this study challenges some widely held assumptions about AfrE, particularly, in regard to the nature of AfrE vowel systems. Although the primary objective of this study is to define and interpret the phonetic and phonemic identities of AfrE vowel systems, defining a methodological framework for future studies is an equally significant goal. This study accentuates the need for more empirical investigation in the study of English (as well as other languages) in the densely multilingual sub-Saharan Africa. The lack of information about the techniques and instruments used in the study of AfrE vowels has been a major shortcoming of previous studies. The lack of empirical evidence pertaining to Lls and L25 in this region, has, needless to say, created impediments and challenges to this study. In order to provide an empirical investigation of AfrE vowel systems, this study has adapted tools of investigation and analysis applied in fields of sociolinguistics, acoustic phonetics, and the social psychology of language. Towards this end, sociolinguistic methods of sampling and data collection have been applied in identifying a relatively homogeneous sample. Moreover, the physical characteristics of vowels realized by AfrE speakers of each regional variety have been quantified using tools adapted from acoustic phonetics. Likewise, vowel perception methods, primarily used in field of the social psychology of language, have been utilized in quantifying vowel perception. Collectively, the theories and methods derived from these fields of study have informed this study immensely. Chapter one defines the parameters of this study. Chapter two provides a background to the study while chapter three delineates the guiding theoretical framework. Chapter four is a discussion on methodology while in chapter five, an analysis of acoustic and perception data is discussed. Conclusions are drawn thereafter in chapter six. 1. 1 Statement of the Problem The existing literature characterizes AfrE vowel systems as homogeneous varieties with regionally distinct qualities. The widely documented comparison of RP and AfrE vowel systems (see Figure 1) suggests a seven-vowel system for West African English (WAfrE), and a five-vowel system for East African English (EAfrE). Hardly any empirical evidence is provided to account for the vowel system variation. Research in AfrE vowel systems is not only limited in scope and methodology but also replete with conflicting generalizations and conclusions. The literature exhibits significant flaws in methodology such that reported findings and conclusions require verification. Taking advantage of available tools of investigation and analysis, this study seeks to accomplish that goal. Adegbija (1994: 53) outlines existing shortcomings in current research: Deficiency in methodology appears to be the principal weakness of most language ...[variation] studies hitherto carried out in sub-Saharan Africa. Some of such studies... appear to be largely impressionistic in approach and sampling techniques and instruments, when indicated are often weak. Adegbija's observation echoes Schmied's (1989) earlier challenge to African English researchers to employ new approaches in data collection and analysis, in order to ensure that studies of English in Africa provide a systematic empirical account. In the study of variant vowel systems of African English, many researchers have failed to acknowledge the dense multilingual complexities of sub-Saharan Africa. The assumed homogeneity of speech communities has led to overgeneralizations that seem to outlook the linguistic complexities prevailing in many sub—Saharan countries where tens or hundreds of diverse indigenous languages are spoken, and English is acquired as a second or third language by a great majority of its speakers. Moreover, the use and function of English in these countries, is limited to official settings. In such dynamic language contact situations, complex patterns of influences and transfer of features from one language to another may be easy to find. However, although the exposition of such patterns and transfers are not within the purview of the current study, we should note that the realization of English vowels in such dense multilingual setting may depend on speaker’s L1, level of education, social network, social motivation among other factors. These factors, especially ethnicity, may vary considering that speakers in a given country may speak related languages or distinctively different ones. Previous studies seem to have overlooked such factors as they generalized regional variations of AfrE. Angogo and Hancock (1984: 74-5) adequately represent this common misperception in the field: Phonology is perhaps the most distinctive characteristic of [AfrE]. .. it is quite possible to generalize about the existence of a ‘West African English’ and ‘East African English’ despite their respective geographical variant forms, in the same way that we speak of an American English’ which in fact consists of many regional and social varieties. Even though a Gambian will say “bot” for “but” when a Ghanaian will say “bet”, it does not mean that there are many [WAfrE’s] rather than one; there is a phonological cohesiveness to all these kinds of English which identify them as African rather than, say, West Indian English, and specifically as West African rather than East African. Considering WAfrE as a homogenous language variety overlooks even the widespread pidgin varieties that many scholars argue may constitute a major input in the formation of Englishes in that region. A similar generalization is echoed by Schmied (1991), who considers the Eastern and Southern African English varieties to be so indistinct as to constitute a common variety referred to as Bantu English. Most studies identify three broad regional varieties of African English: West African English (WAfrE), East African English (EAfi'E), and Southern Afiican English (SAfrE). National varieties such as Nigerian, Kenyan, Ghanaian, Cameroon and Zambian Englishes are also discussed. Only a few studies have discussed the variation of the vowel systems in an attempt to identify the phonological distinctions among these AfrE varieties, but sampling techniques, methods of data collection, and the analysis applied in the research are under described. It is uncertain whether the findings reported in these studies represent distinctions exhibited by a particular social class, age group, gender, or by an entire speech community in the densely multilingual speech communities in sub-Saharan Africa. The aim of this exploratory study is to remedy such methodological flaws by presenting a methodological framework that will at least partly answer the need for more empirical research in multilingual Africa. Such a task can be accomplished by maximizing the advantages of the empirical tools of acoustic phonetics. and sociolinguistic research. In using computer software designed to extract, measure and plot vowels, we seek to verify the widely documented findings regarding the AfrE vowel systems . 1. 2 Background to the study The English language enjoys institutional support in most African countries that were under British colonial rule. Ngugi wa Thiong'o (1984:13) points out that in the colonial and post-colonial era in these countries, “English was the official vehicle and the magic formula to colonial [and neo-colonial] elitedom.” It is still the official language and medium of instruction in schools, colleges and universities. However, English remains a minority language in regard to number of users and frequency of use. African languages remain the dominant languages of informal interactions, even among well-educated AfrE speakers. In language contact situations, features of native languages are bound to be manifested in the realization of the acquired languages. Non-native speakers of English in Africa may perceive and produce English vowels based on the phonological schema of their first language. Consequently, a new form of English, distinctive to that speech community, grows and over a period of time is institutionalized and accepted. However, although the foregoing assertion assumes that speakers of related Lls may have relatively similar perceptions and production of English, it should be stressed that social factors may also influence production and perception of non-native sounds. The regular usage and acceptance of such new varieties of English eventually leads to institutionalization of the Africanized variety and a gradual change of linguisitc roles and attitudes in the speech communities. The Africanized English that was a stigmatized non-standard variety at one time has become a prestigious standard variety and a target language for many new learners of English in the speech community. In an earlier attitudinal study, Mutonya (1997) reported that AfrE speakers readily identify regional varieties of AfrE and attach attitudinal values to sub-varieties of regional AfrE varieties on the basis of degree of accentedness. Kachru (1982245) reported similar attitudinal judgments among Ghanaian speakers of AfrE: educated Ghanaian English is acceptable; but as Sey (1973: 1) warns us, it does not entail competence in speaking RP since in Ghana the type that strives too obviously to approximate to RP is frowned upon as distasteful and pedantic. ln Nigeria the situation is not different from Ghana or India... Many Nigerians will consider as affected or even snobbish any Nigerian who speaks like a native speaker of English. However, there is hardly any empirical evidence that teases out the distinctive linguistic characteristics of each one of these varieties. Besides such attitudinal research, detailed linguistic analysis of the varieties will help in teasing out the defining features of those varieties. By utilizing tools of research that will guarantee greater precision, this study begins that work by initially measuring acoustic qualities of vowel production in each regional variety, followed by a methodical measurement of listeners’ abilities to identify and discriminate those vowels, and finally, a correlation of production and perception results. Correlating speech production and perception will help determine the most probable vowel system in each region. 1. 3 Theoretical Framework This study seeks to identify the nature of vowel systems of a homogeneous sample of university students from Kenya, Ghana and Zimbabwe. Measurements of vowel production and perception are correlated to determine the probable vowel system of each sample. Peterson and Barney (1952) observed that speakers’ pronunciations of vowels within words are influenced by their particular dialectal background. Furthermore, they noted, pronunciation of vowels may differ both in phonetic quality and in measurable characteristics from that produced speakers with other backgrounds. Listeners, likewise, are influenced in their identification of a sound by their past linguistic experience. Lieberman and Blumstein (1993:153) accentuate the importance of psychoacoustic tests in speech perception research: Although it is possible to perform precise analyses of speech signals using electronic instruments and computer programs that effect various mathematical transformations of the signal, these analyses are, in themselves, meaningless. We can never be certain that we have actually isolated the acoustic cues that people use to transmit information to each other unless we run psychoacoustic studies in which human listeners respond to acoustic signals that differ with respect to the acoustic cues that we think are relevant This research is further informed by Lieberman and Blumstein’s (1993:170...) phonetic theory (physiological theory for vowels) that is structured in terms of biological mechanisms that are involved in the production and perception of speech. Contrary to traditional "articulatory" vowel theory that postulate that the phonetic quality of vowels is derived from the position and the height of point of constriction of the tongue, this physiological theory, as demonstrated by Steven and House (1955), postulates that most vowels can be generated by means of many different articulatory patterns, e.g., adjustments of lip opening and total vocal tract length.‘ The only vowels that need particular tongue contours are [i], [u] and [a]. Since that is the case, Jacobson and Ladefoged (1972:93) prefer perception to production in analyzing and synthesizing a speech signal: The nature of some vowel targets is much more likely to be auditory than articulatory. The particular articulatory mechanism that a speaker makes use of to attain a vowel target is of secondary importance only The physiological approach to phonetic theory for vowels recognizes the fact that the shape of the supralaryngeal vocal tract determines the particular acoustic signal, and different speech sounds are specified by different acoustic signals. Since different 8 speakers have different supralyngaeal vocal lengths, human speakers do not attempt to produce the same absolute formant frequency values for the "same" vowel; instead, they produce a set of format frequencies that is frequency-scaled to their approximate supralaryngeal vocal tract length. In vowel perception, Lieberman argues, a listener approximates the length of a speaker's supralaryngeal vocal tract and normalizes it within the parameters of an appropriate vowel space. A human listener has to determine the probable length of the supralaryngeal vocal tract of the speakers he is listening to in order to determine the frequency parameters of the appropriate "vowel" space. Psychoacoustic experiments show that human listeners can make use of various acoustic cues and strategies to effect tract normalization. (1993: 179) Phonetic information produced by the speaker and the phonological knowledge of the listener are inter-linked in a process that reveals to us the authenticity or relevance of the acoustic cues we analyze, in this case, the similarities and differences that we seek to define in the variation of AfrE vowel systems. The physiological approach further states that, since the quantal vowels, ([i], [u], [a]), are maximally distinct, they help define the vowel space within which speakers differentiate the other vowels of English. The quantal vowels have well-defined spectral peaks because of the convergence of two formant frequencies: F1 and F2 converge to yield a central spectral peak at about 1 kHz for [a]. F2 and F3 converge to yield a high frequency peak for [i], F1 and F2 converge to yield a low frequency spectral peak for [u]. (Lieberman and Blumstein 1993: 175) Several psycho-acoustic tests have determined that [i] and [u] produce the lowest errors of all vowels when listeners are asked to identify the vowels. In contrast, the Peterson and Barney (1952) study shows a high rate of confusion occurs between [Ct] and [o ] due to the instability of phonemic /a / and /0 / distinctions in many dialects of English . Are these findings manifested in the study of AfrE vowel systems? Lieberman (1993:182-3) further states that languages do not have the same inventory of peripheral, non-quantal vowels. Speakers of a language may be unable to identify non-quantal vowels of another language with certainty, because vowels seem to be perceived by means of neural acoustic property detectors that respond to particular acoustic signals. Moreover, Lieberman argues, the range of formant frequencies to which these property detectors can potentially respond is delimited by the quantal vowels [i], [u] and [a]. Our response to particular vowel sounds is made possible by neural devices that are "tuned" to particular acoustic signals that reflect the constraints of our speech producing mechanisms. Furthermore, humans partition the possible range of formant frequencies that the human vocal tract can generate differently as they grow up in different linguistic environments. Therefore, the different linguistic and social background of different AfrE speakers will condition the nature of acoustic signals that their neural acoustic devices detect in speech perception. Other studies have contributed to a better understanding of a physiological approach to phonetic theory. Niedzielski (1999: 63) suggests that listeners use a variety of different factors in their perception and interpretation of a speaker's dialect, including social expectations that are influenced by socially constructed beliefs and stereotypes Strand (1999:96) states that, beyond the language-specific phonetics, socially constructed information also affects how we perceive and categorize speech sounds. Some of those influencing factors include dialect background or nationality of the speaker. We expect our speakers and listeners from each region to exhibit such socially 10 determined influences in their speech production and perception, and the results of this present study will be interpreted in light of earlier attitudinal work to begin an account of such influences. l. 4 Scope and Limitations In this exploratory study, only a representative sample will be chosen from each region. Speech samples will be recorded from a population that is controlled for level of education, mother tongue, region and linguistic evidence to be analyzed. A major criticism leveled against previous research efforts in the region is the glaring overgeneralization that seems oblivious to the multilingual complexities of sub-Saharan Africa. Controlling our sample for homogeneity seeks to avoid such a methodological flaw. Respondents are chosen from a population of male and female university students from Kenya (EAfrE), Zimbabwe (SAfrE), and Ghana (WAfrE). A comparative study of production and perception of such a sample will enable us to make concrete claims about the vowel systems of educated speakers in those regions. The implicit target sample of earlier studies was educated AfrE speakers. Angogo and Hancock (1982) categorize the following four types of Englishes spoken Sub-Saharan Africa: Type 1 is the native English of whites and expatriates born or living in Africa; Type 2 is the native English of Africans of racially or linguistically mixed marriages; Type 3 is the non-native English spoken as a second language by well- educated Africans who, by virtue of their academic or professional life, speak English 11 fluently as a second language; Type 4 is pidgin/Creole English, such as Nigerian Pidgin English. Angogo and Hancock conclude that Type 3 is prototypical AfrE. This study focuses on educated AfrE speakers. Although all other varieties mentioned above are relevant in understanding AfrE, the scope of the current study limits us from discussing them comprehensively. Gfirlach ( 1991 :24), argues that pronunciation of AfrE speakers is 'the most reliable test for localizing a speaker.’ Schmied (1991 :57) concurs: the pronunciation of English in Africa is of particular importance because (non- standard) pronunciation features seem to be the most persisitent in African varieties i.e., they are retained even in the speech of the most educated speakers. It is for this reason that we chose phonological evidence over syntactic, lexical, or morphological variation for this study. Kenya, Zimbabwe, and Ghana were chosen specifically because of their almost identical history of colonization by Britain and for the dominant role that English continues to play in the social relations of the citizens of these countries. The Bantu and Kwa language groups were chosen because they belong to the larger Niger-Congo family group. However, we should note that even among these language group there are subtle dialectal differences, but have minimized the differences among Kwa speakers by choosing respondents from the Akan-dialect cluster. Kwa languages are a branch of the Niger-Congo language family spoken by the inhabitants of an area extending along the Atlantic coast of Africa from Céte d'Ivoire to the Nigerian border and including the southern parts of Cote d'Ivoire, Ghana, Togo, and Benin. The Kwa languages, Stewart (1984) argue, include the Akan cluster, with 7 million speakers. The principal members of the Akan cluster are Asante Twi, Akuapem 12 (Akuapim), and F ante in Ghana; Anyi and Baule (2 million) in Cote d'Ivoire; and Ewe (2 million) in southeast Ghana and southern Togo. Our Ghanaian respondents are L1 speakers of the Akan dialect cluster, which comprises Twi , Asante and Fante languages. Some respondents spoke fluently two or three of these languages. Bantu languages are spoken by millions of people in sub-Saharan Africa below a line roughly demarcated by the southern boundaries of Nigeria, Chad, Central African republic, Sudan, Ethiopia and Somalia. 15 of the 37 languages of Africa which have a million or more speakers are Bantu languages (Hinnebuschl989: 451) Kenyan respondents speak the following Bantu languages: Gikuyu, Kikamba, Kitaita and Ekegusii . Most Zimbabweans are Shona and Ndebele speakers although some speak Chinyanja and Chichewa. Suprasegmental variations such as pitch, tone, Advanced Tongue Root (ATR), are not within the scope of the current study. The study focuses only on the onset (steady state) characteristics of vowels. F1 and F2 frequency measurement is the only criteria used in this study to determine acoustic quality of the vowels. The voWels were elicited by requesting respondents to read stimulus word-list adapted from previous studies. 1. 5 Data Collection Two sets of data were collected for this study: acoustic data from speakers and psychoacoustic data from listeners. Acoustic Data: A questionnaire and interview method was used to collect data. The questionnaire (see Appendix A) comprised four sections: biographical information, word list, reading 13 passage and respondent's data on language use. Every interview was tape-recorded. Having provided the interviewer with biographical information, the respondent was then asked to read a list of nineteen words . The word list was adapted from the lexical items used in previous studies (see Figure 1 below), but other lexical items were added to represent vowels and diphthongs missing from Figure 1. The respondent then read a passage that was constructed to incorporate the lexical items in the word list, in order to give each respondent the chance to produce the lexical item in two styles: formal style for the word list, and less formal for the reading passage. For this particular study, word list productions were heavily relied upon, although in a few cases we relied on reading passage productions in cases of audio problems or misreading of words. Such cases were very rare and did not influence our findings. Psychoacoustic data After results of the acoustic analysis were obtained, a perceptual test was carried out. A sample of subjects with the same demographic characteristics as those who read the word list was identified. Speech perception of fifty university students from each region were elicited using Identification Test (IT) and Minimal pair Test (MPT). In each of these two tasks, listeners heard fifty seven minimal pairs of carefully selected monosyllabic words, recorded by an RP speaker. In the IT task, listeners identified each word in every pair, while the MPT entailed determining whether the words in each pair were “same” or “different.” (see Appendix D) 1. 6 Data Analysis The steady state of vowels were extracted from the recordings and frequencies of the first two formants (F 1 and F2) calculated using a computer program (SIGNALYZE, l4 version 3.12) designed for acoustic analysis. The F1 and F2 scores were calculated using linear predictive coding (LPC). Labov (1994) argues that LPC increases accuracy and reliability achieved in acoustic analyses through the use of software methods that apply LPC to the digitized speech wave. The formant measurements were subsequently entered into a computer program (PLOTNIK, version 04) designed for plotting and displaying vowel systems. Results of the psychoacoustic test was calculated to determine the frequency of correct identification of each vowel and discriminations between them in a minimal pair test. Comparisons were made and conclusions drawn. 15 Chapter 2: Background to the Study 2. l. 0 Introduction The background to the study of AfrE regional vowel systems is presented in the form of a review of relevant literature. Previous studies have provided little or no empirical evidence to corroborate observations and conclusions, and subsequently subjecting the studies to accusations of overgeneralizations, methodological flaws and implicit assumption in representation of AfrE vowel systems. 2. 2. 0 Background to the study 2. 2. 1 African English? The term ‘African English’ has been used as a theoretical abstraction by the few scholars who have done research on the forms and functions of English in Anglophone Africa. The most notable publications in this field are Todd (1982), Ladham (1982), Angogo and Hancock (1982), Pride (1982), Kachru (1986), Cheshire (1991), Schmied (1991), Gérlach (1991), and Adegbija (1994). Is it presumptuous to conceive of such an idea as ‘African English,’ taking into account that English-speaking sub-Saharan African countries are densely multilingual and that English is spoken by an educated minority as a second language? Some scholars have attempted to answer this question as follows. Gorlach (1991: 123) carried out tests among native English speakers of British and American origins. He found that these students had no great difficulty in identifying a speaker from Nigeria as African. Although this observation does not provide much evidence regarding the existence of an African English, it vaguely points to the fact there are some distinct linguistic features that are uniquely African, at least in the speech perception of other non-Afiican speakers of English. Schmied (1991 :2) defines African English as : forms of English spoken by African speakers; this does not imply that there is an acknowledged variety or that there are several distinct varieties of the language, nor that these forms are already standardized and codified in any way. ' Schmied’s definition uses the geographical criterion to identify the English variety. It also groups together all forms of English, from the English-based Creoles and Pidgins in West Africa to other Africanized and non-Africanized English varieties spoken in most English speaking regions. In view of the linguistic diversity of Africa, this is a misleading definition. Bokamba (1982:78) reiterates Gorlach’s argument that native speakers of English are able to positively identify an African English utterance. He alludes to distinctive linguistic characteristics that define AfrE: these Englishes share certain properties that can be identified as Africanisms, in that they reflect structural characteristics of African languages. Specifically, these properties can be discovered at all linguistic levels: phonological, morphological, semantic and syntactic. Bokamba’s study focuses on the syntactic and semantic properties of sentences produced by educated Africans. Our area of concern in this study is phonology. The Africanisms defined by Bokamba are, in Lanham’s (19651198) words, ‘deviations from the authentic English’ which arise as a result of the transfer of features and strategies previously known from the mother tongue to the target language. In every major linguistic area of the world where English is learnt as a second or foreign language, Lanham argues, there is a characteristic set of deviations from authentic English, much of which is as a result of transfer from the mother tongue to English. With the passage of 17 time these deviations become institutionalized and give specific identity to the English variety, as is the case for Indian English, African English, Singapore English, and so on. These early descriptive studies played a very important role in laying a foundation for the study of English in Africa. Although evidence does not point to careful empirical analysis of data, such pioneering studies generated a research interest that has culminated in more studies on varieties of African English. In a much more recent development, the first international dictionary of World Englishes (1999) classifies African English as one of the major varieties of English. These studies have presented phonological, syntactic, lexical and morphological evidence in support of the distinctness of an African English variety in relation to RP and other Englishes. However, this study is limited only to phonological evidence. Why focus on phonology? Pronunciation is the ‘most reliable test for localizing a speaker’ (Gorlach 1991224); Schmied's ( 1991:57) agrees: the pronunciation of English in Africa is of particular importance because (non-standard) pronunciation features seem to be the most persistent in African varieties i.e., they are retained even in the speech of the most educated speakers. Gimson (1989:3180 notes that the British phoneme system may pose problems for foreign learners of English. The main problems are identified as: the tense-lax opposition of the close vowels /i/-/I/, /u/-/u/; the existence of a long central vowel / 3/; and phonemic length. Schmied, a widely cited authority in the field, makes three contrasting generalizations about the AfrE and RP vowel systems. (i) There is a length difference in vowels. The African English short vowels are longer and more peripheral than in RP, especially the closed /I/, /U/ or /A/. 18 (ii) The central vowels /A/ (but), and /3/ (bird) are avoided and tend towards open positions [ i, a]. (iii) Diphthongs tend to be monophthongized. Schmied's characterization of the AfrE vowel system is broadly acknowledged in the literature, and many studies have followed with similar generalized descriptions of AfrE’s regional vowel systems. However, such generalizations are yet to be corrobated by compelling empirical evidence as proposed in this study. 2. 2. 2 AfrE’s Regional Vowel Systems Example RP WafrE EafrE Bead i i i Bid 1/ / Bade e e Bed ' e e \ Bird 3 Bad a\ a a Bud A Bod o 0 Board 0 > o Bode o ............. 0 Pull 0 pool u;u\u Figure 1 - Adaptedl representation of Schmied's classical comparison of RP, WAfrE, and EAfrE ' Phonetic symbols have been adapted to reflect a more consistent phonetic transcription. 19 Although not included in Schmied’s illustration of vowel systems, the following diphthongs have been investigated; [aI]buy, [au]bout, [3i]boy, [ia]year, [03] sure and [89] hair. Schmied’s (1991 :59) characterization of AfrE diphthongs states with a sense of finality that, “all the centring diphthongs (lial, leel, lua/ tend to be pronounced as opening diphthongs or double monophthongs (lia, ea, ual)” Figure 1 contains background information that is essential to this study. The primary literature on AfrE vowel variation utilizes information contained in this illustration. Figure 1 compares and contrasts WAfrE and EAfrE vowel systems with those of the RP system. AfrE researchers, including Schmied, have made general claims that the SAfrE vowel system is identical to EAfrE, considering the common Bantu substratum of L2 English speakers of these varieties. Whether such conclusions represent every group in this ethnically and linguistically diverse region has yet to be proven empirically. A quick glance at Figure 1 shows that WAfrE is portrayed as a seven-vowel system whereas EAfrE is a five—vowel system. Previous studies attribute variation in the AfrE vowel systems to such phonological processes as simplification leading to loss of quantity distinction, reduction leading to mergers, and decentralization of the central vowels. Some major claims that have been made in the literature concerning the nature of the vowel systems of AfrE are stated below. Schmied (1991) attributes a major source of AfrE variation to the ‘deviation’ in the production of the RP long central vowel [3] (as in bird) by speakers of each regional 20 speech community. Schmied claims that [3] backs to [o] in WAfrE, fronts and lowers to [a] or fronts to [e] in EAfrE, while in SAfrE it fronts to [e]. Todd (1984) provides further examples of backing and fronting of the central vowels in the realignment of RP vowels in the WAfrE vowel system, e.g., the /A/ phoneme which occurs in the word but is realized as [0]. Additionally, the la/ phoneme as in hat, rat, bad is realized as [(1]. Both these claims are consistent with what Schmied presents in Figure 1. Bokamba (1992:21) also discusses the reduction in the AfrE vowel system to five or seven vowels; /i, e, a, o, u / and perhaps /0/ and /8/ resulting from vowel mergers in such pairs and triplets as bit/beat, had/hard,full/fool, and cut/court/caught. Bokamba further argues that individual items may be realized variously: bed as [bed] or [bed], bird as [bed] or [bad]. These claims are also consistent with Schmied's, as shown in Figure l. Angogo and Hancock (1980: 72f) contend that, in discussing regional varieties of African English, it is important to note that each variety consists of several levels of style and acceptability. Nevertheless, like any other regional varieties of English, they contend, AfrE varieties exhibit a homogeneity between English as spoken in West Africa and English as spoken in East Africa. Furthermore, in their judgment, the model for AfrE is the non-native English spoken fluently as a second language by African people who have learnt an African language first in life, but, because of their education or profession, have grown up hearing and using English daily. These social groups speak English as well as, or maybe even better than, their mother tongues. Angogo and Hancock (1980: 72) are explicit about the phonology: 21 [AfrE] phonology is already becoming well established. This is evident. from the fact that speakers whose native languages have a wide range of vowel sounds nevertheless retain [AfrE] phonology; Igbo distinguishes between tense and lax [u] and [u], and [i] and [I] but Igbo speakers of [WAfrE] will not as a rule differentiate between the pairs fool/full or sheep/ship. Temne speakers contrast [a], [a] in their language, but for them, faster will still be pronounced [fasta], not [fa:sto]. These claims about WAfrE, are also consistent with Schmied's. In short, Angogo and Hancock (74-5) argue specifically that regional varieties exist in spite of considerable internal variation. They argue that phonology is perhaps the most distinctive characteristic of AfrE. Furthermore, as stated earlier in this study, they insist that it is quite possible to generalize about the existence of a ‘West African English’ and ‘East African English,’ in spite of their respective geographical variant forms, in the same way that we speak of an ‘American English’ that consists of many regional and social varieties. Whether 'phonological cohesiveness' condenses varieties and subvarieties of Englishes spoken in particular regions into one geographical variety (WAfrE, EAfrE, or SAfrE) is an issue that continues to be debated. This paper seeks not to be embroiled in the debate, but proposes extensive empirical studies (analogous to the one carried out in this study), focusing on well-defined homogeneous populations in’the regions as a beginning "phonetic" approach to the problem. Banda (1996) also laments the methodological flaws in research on AfrE, and the paucity of phonological indices of educated Africans, whose speech is the target language of most learners of English as a second language in sub-Saharan Africa. Banda concurs with Jibril’s (1986) claim that educated AfrE with a fairly African accent is usually the accepted target language norm for most English learners in Africa, while heavily ethnically marked accents are not. 22 Consequently, Banda (1996:68) further argues that describing AfrE from the perspective of English phoneme systems, as previous research has done, is not only erroneous, but it also distorts data. thorough and useful descriptions of New Englishes are unavailable, particularly the phonological indices of the [Target Language] Educated AfrE. Studies that have attempted to do comparative analyses of vowel and consonant systems in African Englishes are usually based on [Non-native English varieties], but even lack the phonological data and sociocultural awareness that is crucial for the sort of analysis required... researchers have yet to identify, let alone describe the distinguishing acoustic correlates in AfrE . Banda may be criticized for generalizations, but certainly not for identifying the problem that confronts research in AfrE. Our exploratory study addresses the phonological uncertainities and gaps discussed by Angogo and Banda. Even Josef Schmied, a leading authority in this field, acknowledges these shortcomings when discussing variant forms of the RP long central vowel, which in his opinion, ‘is the primary parameter that defines regional variation in AfrE.’ He cautions: but their [RP I3 / realizations] tendencies are not uniform in a region, neither across all ethnic groups (Igbo speakers tend towards le/ and Yoruba towards /a/ ...) nor across the lexicon. Note that he builds this variation into Figure 1. The present study begins with the system described in Figure 1 as a basis for reexamining the regional variation of vowel systems of Bantu (in Kenya and Zimbabwe) and Kwa (in Ghana) speakers. 2. 2. 2. 1 West African English (WAfrE) English is the official language in six West African countries.2 Todd (1982) identifies the main types of English spoken in the region as: 2 English is the official language in Gambia, Sierra Leone, Liberia, Ghana, Nigeria and Cameroon. 23 (i) Pidginized and Creolized variety: other-tongues creoles of Liberia (Merico) and Sierra Leone (Krio) and of the Krio—speaking settlers in Gambia, Nigeria, Cameroon and Equatorial Guinea. Pidgin Englishes of the coastal regions and of many urban communities. (ii) Second language English that is acquired in schools and strongly influenced by the mother tongue(s) of speakers, also known as “ broken English.” (iii) Standard WAfrE, which with the exception of Liberia " is equated with British (RP) norms. It is spoken as a second language by educated African speakers. (iv) the English of expatriates ( mainly American, British, Indian and Lebanese). Our interest is in type (iii). Like others, Todd argues that there are distinct characteristics of WAfrE: in spite of regional and educational differences, certain generalizations can be made about pronunciations of WAE largely because West African languages are fairly similar in structure. Similarity of the structure of the West African languages, as claimed by Todd above, is a highly contentious issue. Even among the Niger-Congo languages there are structural differences; some have SOV word order whereas others have SVO; moreover, vowel systems are not uniform in this family group (Williamson .1989). Here we summarize Todd's description of the WAfrE vowel system: (a) WAfrE has fewer vowel contrasts than RP. It utilizes 7 vowels and 3 or 4 diphthongs. (b) Central vowels and centring diphthongs are virtually nonexistent. Narrow diphthongs in bade and goat tend to be monophthongized to [bed] and /got/. Words which end in [a] in RP have [a] in WAfrE, e.g. hair [hea]. (c) Long central vowel fronting: bird is realized as [bed]. ((1) [A ] is replaced by [0] : [bAd] is realized as [bod] (e) [a] is realized as [a] 24 Trudgill and Hannah (1985), identify a WAfrE3 vowel system that contains seven vowels and three diphthongs. This system has no central vowels and lacks tense/ lax contrasts especially among high and low vowels. /i/ bid, bead4 /e/ bade la / bed, burn /cr/ bad, bard ID/ bod, bud /o/ boat /u/ pool, pull lai/ buy ID i /———— boy /au/ ———- bout Figure 2 - WAfrE Vowel System as presented by Trudgill and Hannah This analysis is similar to other findings reported in other studies; it can be read as Figure 1 “backwards” - from phonemes to “word classes” Writing about varieties of English in Cameroon, Todd (1982) argues that vocalic contrasts of the seven-vowel system are reflected in Cameroon English. Consequently, many Cameroonians will hardly distinguish between /i/ and /I/ as in bead and bid, between lo/ and /D/ as in board and bad, or between /a/ and / a/ as in bard and bad. Central vowels and centring dipthongs are avoided with the result that words like year and hair are realized as lia/ and /he/. The seven-vowel system for the Cameroonian vernaculars (Lamso and Bulu), and in effect Cameroon English, is shown in the vowel chart in Figure 3. 3 By WafrE, the writers stress that they refer to ‘ varieties that are unarnbigously English, particularly those spoken in Ghana, Nigeria and Sierra Leone.’ (102) " Trudgill and Hannah’s lexical items are substituted with Schmied’s (Figure 1) without altering the vowels intended. Similar changes have been made whenever different lexical items are used to represent vowels already represented in Shmied’s figure. 25 The vowel chart also reflects the vowel system of West African Pidgin English, as documented by Dwyer (1969 ) and Schneider (1966). r u / Figure 3 - Todd's representation of the vowel system of Cameroon English Comparing RP and educated Ghanaian English, Sey (1973) provides the following contrastive evidence: RP Ghanaian English Bad Ia/ Bard /CI/ /(1/ Bade /e/ Bod /D/ /D/ Bird /3/ /8/ Pool /u/ \ Pull /u/ ‘ /u/ Beat /i/ \ Bid /I/ /1/ Figure 4 - Sey's Comparison of RP and Ghanaian Vowel Systems Two claims made by Sey are particularly important to our study : (a) Sey claims the vowel /3 / as in bird is fronted and realized as /8/ contrary to claims made in other studies (b) Sey claims that the vowel /A/ as in the words bud is realised as either /D/ or /8/. However, Sey qualifies this observation with another baffling claim: /A/ does not occur in L1, but the most likely substitute for it [in L2] would be /Ct/ and not /0/ or /8/ (147). But he does not speculate on why, in fact, [D] and [8] actually occur. In a comparative study of normative English pronunciations in Cameroon and Nigeria by educated speakers, Bobda (1995) claims that educated Nigerian and Cameroonian Englishes are homogeneous across regional and ethnic boundaries. He argues that speakers of sub-varieties of English usually exhibit norms of the national variety; for instance, speakers of a language which may have vowel contrasts similar to RP's will neutralize the contrasts in their English productions in conformity with the national norm. To support his claim, Bobda gives an example of Hausa language which has contrastive [i] and [I] vowels, and yet speakers will not make a contrast in their production of English beat and bit. Bobda illustrates the similarities between Nigerian and Cameroonian Englishes as shown in Figures 5 and 6. 27 RP Ni gE and CamE Beat/i/ / /i/ Bid /I/ Pool/u/ /u/ Pull /u/ Bad /a/ /a/ Bard / CI/ Bod /D/ / 0/ board/ 0/ bud /A/ goat Ion / sure lue/ Figure 5 - A comparison of RP, Nigerian and Cameroonian Vowel Systems. A spelling pronunciation of RP schwa into different segments in both varieties is represented in Figure 6. Bobda criticizes the overgeneralizations of studies of West African Englishes for apparently misrepresenting the facts. He rejects the claim by Hancock and Angogo, echoed by Gorlach (1991:135), Schmied (1991 :61) and others, that the pronunciation of bird as [bod] is a feature of WAfrE. He contends that bird has the vowel [8] as in the words girl and shirt, presumably distinct from [c], as in made. Bobda claims that most of features identified as West Africanisms are indeed Nigerianisms, which have different realizations in CamE. Although Bobda’s study 28 distances itself from others, the empirical basis of his findings is as doubtful as the others and continues to illustrate the murky state of research in AfrE. RP Ni gE and CamE a sofa C statement i visible e O docth Ll status Figure 6 - The Realization of RP Schwa in Nigerian and Cameroonian Englishes Bobda’s assumptions are illustrative of the confusion that non-empirical studies pose to the field. For instance, the claim that Cameroonian English is a “ some variety of Nigerianisms” poses more questions than answers. Which Nigerianisms is Bobda referring to? Do these characteristics transcend ethnic, social and linguistic boundaries? Such concerns mitigate for a more empirical investigation of languages in densely multilingual sub-Saharan region. ’ 2. 2. 2. 2 East African English (EAfrE) Angogo and Hancock (1982) state that while West Africa had little exposure to native speakers of English, East Africa had a considerable number of native speakers who had a greater influence in government and teaching during the colonial and the post- independence era. Due to the continued reinforcement of British standards in the schools, EAfrE never strayed far from the prestige dialect of England . They identify four types of English in the region: 29 (i) Native English (NE) of whites and expatriates. (ii) NE of Africans of racially or linguistically mixed marriages. (iii) Non native English (NNE) spoken fluently as a second language. The speech variety of the educated, mid and higher status Africans, ‘...it is the speech of this group, which may be taken as the norm of the varieties of EAE.’ (308) (iv) NNE imperfectly used as the foreign language by speakers 'who have little knowledge of English. The samples in this study were drawn from type (iii) speakers who are educated and have a higher rate of contact with native speakers of English. The distinct vowel characteristics of EAfrE are: (a) Vowel raising : ran is realized as [ran] (b) Contrast of /a/ and / A/ is collapsed to [a] (c) Tensed and lax vowels are not distinguished. Hancock and Todd (1987) provide additional cases of vowel mergers in educated EafrE: (a) Little distinction is made between vowels in: bear and bid (usually realized as [I] ) bad and bed (usually realized as [e] ) Far and for and fore (realized as [a] ) Pool and pull ( realized as [u] ) (b) Diphthongs are monophthongised lei/ is realised as [e], so that bade and bed are often indistinguishable. As stated above, there is no empirical evidence to support the above analysis. 30 2. 2. 2. 3 Southern African English (SAfrE) Todd and Hancock (1987) identify nine Southern African English speaking countries: Angola, Zambia, Malawi, Lesotho, Zimbabwe, Mozambique, Namibia, Botswana and Swaziland. However, Angola and Mozambique have a much stronger contact with Portuguese than English to be proper members of the set. Namibia's contact with Afrikaans also disqualifies it. Todd and Hancock (1987:431) further argue that: There are many similarities between the English of east and southern Africa, the most marked of which is the tendency to raise the vowel in back so that back and beg differ mainly in terms of the final consonant. They also note that SAfrE vowel length distinctions are rarely preserved, leading to such mergers as: /i/ and /I/ so that bead and bid are both realized as [bid] /a/ and / 8/ so that bad and bed are realized as [ bad] /D/ and/A/ so that bod and bud are realized as [de] /u/ and /U/ so that pool and pull are realized as [pul] Moreover, central vowels are avoided. Schwa is replaced by /a/ and / 3/ by /e/. The absence of the SAfrE vowels in Figure 1 is due to the fact that the available literature analyzes the phonological characteristics of native speakers of English in the region. Literature on Black non-native speakers is scarce and sketchy. Angogo and Hancock (1982) argue that there is a phonological similarity between SAfrE and EAfrE varieties since both varieties are influenced by indigenous languages that share a common Bantu substratum. 31 This study seeks to verify the assumptions about these broad varieties against vowel production and perception results of relatively homogeneous samples from Ghana, Kenya and Zimbabwe. 32 Chapter Three: Theoretical Framework 3. 1. 0 Introduction This chapter discusses some basic theoretical premises that inform this study on issues pertaining to vowel perception and production, and on the definition of phoneme. By adapting some of the theoretical positions that are principally derived from empirical research on monolingual and bilingual populations, this exploratory study seeks to determine whether such assumptions apply to multilingual speakers. The Native Language Magnet Theory, the principal guiding theory of the current research, is particularly relevant, for it considers the perception of non-native speech. 3. 2. 0 Theoretical Framework Theoretical assumptions discussed in this section are derived from previous research in linguistics and psycholinguistics. Of particular interest to this study are attempts to correlate speech production and perception (the phonetics/phonology interface). Taking into account the nonexistence of similar empirical research in AfrE vowel systems, this study cautiously adapts theoretical assumptions based on the relationship between speech production and perception which are drawn from studies that have primarily focused on monolingual and bilingual speakers. This exploratory study, as stated above, targets multilingual speakers of English as a non-native language. Two particular theories are central to our analysis and correlation of AfrE vowel production and perception. The Acoustic Theory of Speech Production provides an overview of some basic assumptions in acoustic analysis as presented in chapter five; the Native Language Magnet Theory, as outlined below, is our principle guiding theory in 33 our understanding and interpretation vowel perception as discussed in chapter four. Prior to outlining these theories, some general theoretical assumptions about speech production and perception are discussed. 3. 2.1 Some General Theoretical Assumptions (a) Assumptions about AfrE Central to this study is the assumption that African English is a very general term that refers to diverse social, regional and ethnic sub-varieties of English in East, West and Southern Africa. Unlike previous studies that assumed homogeneity of broad regional varieties of AfrE, this study assumes those varieties are diversely influenced by such social and linguistic factors as level of education and ethnicity of these multilingual speakers. Although RP vowel production is used as stimuli in listeners’ speech perception tests in this exploratory study, it is assumed that RP is no longer the primary source of input in the formation of varieties of African English. African Englishes are the predominant target languages for new English speakers in the continent as educational institutions have been Africanized. Unlike in the colonial and early pre-independence era when RP speakers dominated the teaching core in teacher institutions of learning, and RP was the target language for new English learners, AfrE speaking teachers have dominated the classrooms , and consequently varieties of AfrE are target language for English learners. Moreover, RP continues to loose its dominant and prestigious role, as more communities place more emphasis in the growth of indigenous national languages. The expanding roles and social acceptance of Pidgin continues to diminish the dominance of European languages in Africa. For instance, West African Pidgin English (WAPE) has 34 been so much intertwined with English in daily usage that its impact on WAfrE has long been overlooked. Although the impact of WAPE on WAfrE is not within the scope of the current study, Dwyer (1969) and Schneider (1966) have reported that WAPE has a seven vowel system. The similarity of WAPE and WAfrE's seven-vowel system, is an interesting topic worth of serious consideration. (b) Assumptions about Vowels Systems in Some African Languages Having conducted extensive research in African languages in western and southern African, including a reputable collaboration with numerous linguists in sub- Saharan Africa, David Westermann and Ian Ward wrote a classic phonetic guide designed for students interested in the study of African languages. Using x-rays to determine tongue position during vowel production, and a Cardinal vowel schema to compare and analysis vowel systems, these phoneticians observed that many African languages have seven-vowel systems, a few have more than seven, while others have five or six vowels only. Ladefoged (1993) observes that phoneticians have extensively used the arbitrary reference points of cardinal vowel system as a scale for describing a wide variety of languages. He points out that the system consists of eight cardinal vowels, “evenly spaced around the outside of the possible vowel area and designed to act as a fixed reference point for phoneticians” (219). Although critics have noted the seemingly arbitrary nature of the vowel representation and the confusion over whether vowels are described on the chart in terms of tongue height or in terms of acoustic properties, Ladefoged ( 221) concludes: 35 Despite all these problems, the cardinal vowel system works fairly successfully. It has allowed the vowels of a large number of languages and dialects to be described with far greater precision than has any other method. The methods used by these phoneticians may have been the most appropriate tools to ensure precision in vowel comparisons and characterization, which may not withstand the test of time in modern studies, but may still represent assumptions worth considering, in a field lacking in empirical research. However, these phoneticians have emphasized more on the physical characteristics of the vowel. The non-physical attributes are not investigated as the current study has attempted to in the perception tests. There are eight cardinal vowel reference points commonly used by phoneticians (see Appendix A’(i)) and Westermann and Ward have shown how the RP vowels fit into the scheme ( See Appendix A’(ii)) . The vowel chart points 1 through 8 indicate the limits of possible vowel quality. For example, point 1/i/, represents the highest and most front possible vowel and its relation to the other front vowels. Westermann and Ward make the following observations about vowels of African languages: (a) Cardinal vowel No. 1 /i/: They claim that every language contains a vowel of i-type vowel. (b) /e/ and / 8 / : A large number of African languages contain two /e/ sounds ; a close one (Cardinal no. 2) and an open one (Cardinal no. 3). They claim that the English vowel in the word bed lies between the two. In some languages, for instances Zulu, the two vowels are allophones. 36 (0) Most languages have /a/ type vowel; in some languages, they further argue, it is nearer to the front /a/ (Cardinal no. 4) and in others it is nearer the back /13/ (Cardinal No.5) African /a/ is rarely like English /a/ in man. (d) Like /e/ and /a/, /o/ and /o/ are two ./0/ sounds; close and open /0/ closer to Cardinal No. 6 and /o/ Cardinal No. 7. (e) Cardinal No. 8 /u/ can be long or short without changing quality. These assumptions apply to a wide range of diverse languages, and only empirical research will verifying the conclusions made. In the meantime, they can assumed to be a rough depiction of vowels of Lls discussed in this study. Note that Westermann and Ward explicitly illustrate a peripheral vowel system lacking in central vowels, consistent with the acoustic analysis discussed later in the study. Do the perception results discussed in Chapter five point towards a Native Language Magnet effect? (c) Assumptions About Speech Production and Perception Some of the general assumptions concerning the correlation between speech production and perception are outlined below. Kenstowicz (1994) argues that finding a proper balance between production and perception of speech sounds of any language remains a central concern of linguistic theory. Such a theoretical balance is essential, Kenstowicz further argues, considering the limitations of the vocal and auditory organs in speech production and perception: the phonological categories we do find empirically attested are constrained by the vocal tract and the human auditory system anatomical apparatus not specifically evolved for the articulation and perception of language. Phonological distinctions and categorizations display gaps that appear arbitrary from a purely abstract, classificatory point of view but seem to reflect contingencies of the articulatory and acoustic systems that realize language in speech. (1994:136) 37 Furthermore, generative phonologists have developed a model for the representation of speech sounds that is based on a close relation between phonetics and phonology. The Articulator Model, Kenstowicz argues, postulates that articulators play a central role not only in the production but also in the perception of speech sounds. The underlying assumption is that phonetics represents the physical realization of abstract linguistic categories. A similar theoretical assumption is contained in Lieberman and Mattingly’s (1985) Motor Theory of Speech Perception, which hypothesizes that listeners’ interpretations of acoustic signals are guided by articulatory gestures in the sense that a variety of acoustic cues for a given feature point to a particular articulatory gesture. In Lieberman’s ( 1993) term, this is vocal tract normalization, a case in which human listeners (in the case of vowel perception) have to determine the probable length of the supralaryngeal vocal tract of the speakers they are listening to in order to determine the frequency parameters of the appropriate vowel space. Experiments conducted by speech scientists studying speech perceptual developments among infants have indicated that human beings are innately endowed with special devices for the perception of speech that differ from the neural and cognitive equipment used to perceive other sounds. Moreover, Lieberman (1990) reports that experiments point to the fact that listeners perceive speech categorically; that is , listeners’ ability to discriminate sounds from two different categories, such as /i/ and /e/, should be relatively easy, but discriminating two different tokens of /i/ from the same speaker should be difficult. 38 Listeners perceive the former as two distinct categories, but as a single sound category in the latter case. Another theoretical assumption that echos a principal premise of sociolonguistic studies is the fact that perception and production of speech are influenced by linguistic and nonlinguistic factors. Speech perception research has shown that adults have difficulties perceiving and discriminating many nonnative contrasts. Best et al (1988), for instance, argue that language environment influences developmental speech perception, and that adult listeners perceive non-native sound contrasts based on a schema developed from their native language (see the Native Language Magnet theory below). Language perception and production are intimately related and are, therefore, difficult to separate operationally, considering that every speaker is simultaneously a listener, and every listener is at least potentially a speaker. 3. 3 Assumptions About Non-Native Speech Perception. Early theories that sought to define the relationship between speech perception and production devoted considerable attention in seeking to explain where in the human brain production and perception of speech took place. While some theorists argued that the functions were completely separated, other theorists, motivated by Broca’s and Wemicke’s discovery (of the distinct types of aphasia caused by lesions at different sites of the brain), held that production is localized on one part of the brain and perception in another, although the functions were interconnected. However, later studies showed the relationship was more complex than that presented by the classical theorists. 39 In this section theoretical assumptions relating to the perception of non-native sounds are discussed. Relevant to the current research is the perception of vowels of an acquired second language that are diverse from the first language of multilingual subjects. Identifying a correlation between speakers’ production and hearers’ perception of vowel variants, will help us determine the phonetic and phonemic properties of AfrE vowel systems. In discussing perception of non-native speech contrasts, Best et al (1988) propose that any explanation of language-specific effects on speech perception should take into account the relation of phonetic properties to phonemic contrasts. Best and colleagues postulate several hypotheses to explain non-native speech perception. Unlike the general assumptions outlined in the previous section, the following hypotheses provide a more linguistically-oriented explanation of the nature of non-native perceptions. The central premise of the following hypotheses is that the native phonological system of the listener plays a prominent role in perception of non-native sound contrasts. These hypotheses are reported in Best et al (1988). The Specific Phonological Relevance Hypothesis, reported in Werker et al (1984), postulates that the more competent infants become in their native language, the less their ability to discriminate contrasting non-native sounds becomes, especially if those sounds do not have corresponding native phonological contrasts. The Allophonic Experience Hypothesis, presented by Tees and Werker (1984), argues that listeners are able to discriminate contrasting non-native phones if their native language contains such similar allophonic variants. For instance, English-speaking adults are able to discriminate, especially after perceptual training, nonnative contrasts in which 40 members occur as allophonic variants in English, e.g., Hindi [d"]-[t"], Spanish utterance initial prevoiced versus voiceless unaspirated [b]-[p]. In contrast, listeners have persistent difficulties with many nonnative contrasts in which one or both members fail to occur allophonically in the (English) native language (Tees & Werker 1984). Werker and Logan (1985) present the Phonemic Perception Hypothesis, which assumes that during speech perception listeners who have acquired a phonological system of their native language perceive incoming sounds phonemically and assimilate them to phonemic categories of their native language. Best et al argue: assimilation may take place regardless of whether those sounds are native or normative and regardless of whether they actually occur allophonically or are simply phonetically similar to some native category ( 1988: 347). Such non-native speech perceptions, they further contend, reveals that when phonemic perception (assimilation) occurs, it may fall into the following three different categories: (a) Single-category assimilation: Contrasting phones are assimilated as variants of a single native category. (b) Opposing-category assimilation: Phones are assimilated as opposing members of a native phonological contrast. (c) Category-goodness difference assimilation: one member is better assimilated to a more phonetically similar category than the other. (d) Non-assimilation: both members are phonetically dissimilar from any native categories and therefore not assimilated. 41 Categories a, b, and c involve perception of phonemically relevant information. Non- assimilated contrasts should be perceived in terms of their auditory or phonetic characteristics. Most contrasts in the world’s languages fall under the first three classes. AfrE speakers learn English as a second or third language after the phonologies of their first languages are well developed. It will interesting to test whether the phonemic perceptions and assimilations outlined in the above hypotheses and assumptions apply to our sample. 3. 4 The Native Language Magnet Theory The Native Language Magnet theory claims that vowels of the native language are stored as prototypes or ideal exemplars of a given category. Further, these prototypes are said to hold a special status in speech perception in that they act as perceptual magnets, thus minimizing differences between the prototype and surrounding stimuli. Kuhl (1993) reports on various studies that indicated that infants are innately predisposed with language-general abilities that initially enable them to perceptually partition a series of sounds at the places that the world’s languages divide the series of stimuli into phonetic categories, rather than at arbitrary places. Furthermore, these initial abilities do not depend on linguistic experience, since infants have shown the same effect for foreign sounds they have never heard. On the other hand, research shows that linguistic experience transforms the language-general perceptual abilities into language- specific ones, as infants begin to acquire words, or as adults encounter difficulty in discriminating certain foreign-language contrasts. Infants are thus born with an excellent capacity to resolve the acoustic differences between speech sounds. 42 Studies from laboratory experiments conducted by Kuhl (1993: 125-6) reveal that: Prototypes (which, for these purposes, 1 am defining as exceptionally “good” instances of members of categories, ideal exemplars) play a unique role in speech perception. Phonetic category prototypes function like perceptual magnets for other stimuli... they attract nearby members of the category, rendering them perceptually similar to the category prototype. .. the magnet effect is obtained only for the prototypes of native- language categories. The results have led to the development of the Native Language Magnet (NLM) theory, which describes how innate factors and experience with a specific language interact in the development of speech perception. Linguistic experiences in different cultures (linguistic environments) result in magnets that differ in number and location of vowel space for speakers and listeners growing up listening to different languages. Kuhl shows that 3 six-month old infants growing up in an English, Swedish, and Japanese environment will have language specific magnets that differ in location and number of vowels in the vowel space. Of central interest to the current study is the NLM‘s explanation of adults’ acquisition of a second language. Kuhl (19932131) argues: The native-language categories of the listener interfere with the ability to perceive certain phonetic distinctions in the new language. The proximity principle again holds: the nearer a new sound is to a nativeolanguage magnet the more it will be assimilated by it, making the new sound indistinguishable from the native-language sound. Phonologists have argued that the phonetic categories of ones’ native language form some sort of “sieve” through which phonetic units of newly acquired language must pass (Trubetzkoy, 1939). NLM provides a potential mechanism by which this could come about. Werker and Polka (1993) report that research with adults has shown that experience with a particular language leads to a decreased perception of at least some non-native phonetic contrasts and an enhanced perception of native phonetic contrasts. Therefore, adults will have difficulties in discriminating phonetic contrasts that are not used in their native language, although they have some ability to do so. Adult speech 43 perception is organized to process the native language with the least effort and greatest efficiency. In cross-language adult vowel perception, Werker and Polka report, evidence shows that effects of language experience are more apparent in the identification rather than in the discrimination of sounds. 3. 5 Acoustic Theory of Speech Production Having discussed our guiding theory in speech perception, a theoretical framework for analysis of speech production is presented in this section. Jusczyk’s (1986) articulation of the theory is our primary reference in this section. The human vocal apparatus is a complex system involving many different sets of muscles to control the actions of variousarticulators involved in the production of speech. Different sets of articulators determine the nature of sound produced. The vocal apparatus generates complex acoustic waveforms. Each speech sound that starts as a pressure wave generated by the lungs has a source that generates an acoustic wave form filtered by the vocal tract as voiced or voiceless. The supralaryngeal tract varies in size and shape, resulting in different acoustic filtering characteristics. For instance, one can narrow the vocal tract in one location while widening it in another as when producing a vowel resulting in changes in acoustic filtering characteristics. Therefore, the physical sound that is realized is a product of several factors. Jusczyk (1986:27-3f) defines those factors such as source spectrum S(f), vocal tract transfer function T(f), and the radiation characteristic R(f); all interact in the production of any given speech sound. Source spectrum consists of components at multiples of the fundamental frequency. The amplitude of the components decreases by about 12 dB per octave at high frequencies. T(f) relates to the filtering characteristics of the vocal tract. It changes with the shape of the vocal tract from one articulatory position to another. The shape of the vocal tract will determine which components of the source spectrum will be reinforced and which ones will be suppressed. The resonant frequencies favored by a particular vocal tract configuration are called formants, and they will appear as formant peaks in the transfer function. R (f) describes the relation between the volume velocity at month opening and sound pressure at distance from the lips. The radiated sound pressure P(f) is a product of all the three components: P(f) = S(f) x T(f) x R(f). The acoustic filtering characteristics of the vocal tract are critical in determining which speech sound emerges during an utterance. As the vocal tract is narrowed at some location during articulation, its resonance characteristic changes. 3. 5. 1 Vowels Vowels can be distinguished by reference to the frequency values for the first two formants. The shared articulatory features of members of a class can be another criteria for distinguishing vowels. Such a division can be made based on the point of the narrowest constriction in the vocal tract, back vowels such as [u], [o], [o ]and [a] are produced with the tongue raised or lowered at the back of the oral cavity. For central vowels, tongue height is modified in the midpalate region, whereas for front vowels the tongue is raised or lowered front of the palate. 45 Degree of closure gives the distinction between open and closed vowels; amount of lip rounding distinguishes rounded and unrounded vowels. There are also monophthongs and diphthongs. Acoustic analysis of AfrE vowel systems presented in Chapter five is defined and interpreted on the basis of Juscyzk’s analysis presented above. 3. 6. 0 Assumptions About Phonemic Identity 3. 6. 1 Introduction While the previous sub-sections discussed AfrE vowel production, this sub- section attempts to illuminate the phonemic shape of variant vowel systems based on listeners’ perception of RP vowels. Although AfrE vowel perception data is the main concern of this chapter, a sketchy history of efforts towards identifying and analyzing phonemes may help contextualize this study relative to earlier AfrE vowel studies discussed in Chapter Two. Conclusions presented later in this chapter are based on a correlation of empirically derived vowel production and perception results. The overview presented here is far from being a comprehensive account of the development of phonemic theory, but it tries to provide a description of some defining stages in the ongoing attempts to present a precise analysis of variation in both physical and abstract terms. 3. 6. 2 On Defining a phoneme: A brief historical overview A historical overview of descriptive and analytic attempts over the years at interpreting phonetic and phonological (or phonemic) identity in language studies may provide a better understanding of the phonemic investigation discussed in this chapter. 46 Outlined below are studies that have significantly contributed to the formulation of phonemic theory and the refinement of methods of phonemic identification. The relevance of such an overview to this study arises from shared objectives, in a very general sense, that is, interest in the interpretation of phonetic and phonological identity. Moreover, a significant contribution of this study lies in methodology: attempts at identifying L2 (or even L3) phonemic variables from multilingual speakers by adapting methods of analysis traditionally used in the investigation of monolingual speech communities. 3. 6. 2. l Phonetic and Phonemic Identity Trubetzskoy and the Prague school phonologists were instrumental in defining the phoneme as a complex phonological unit realized by the sounds of speech. Earlier scholars had viewed the phoneme as a transcriptional device. A phoneme, argued the Prague school, was composed of a number of separate distinctive features with each distinctive feature standing in opposition to another feature or its absence in at least one other phoneme in the language. Adapting Saussurean theoretical concepts, these scholars defined speech sounds as belonging to the physical parole while the phoneme belonged to the abstract langue. As a result of the work of the Prague school, R.H. Robins argues, the phoneme concept “became one of the fundamental elements of linguistic theory as a whole, and of the scientific description and analysis of language” (1990: 226). Ideas postulated by the American structuralists marked the next landmark in the study of phonemes. Leonard Bloomfield’s physicalist view of language analysis and Edward Sapir’s psychological school marked the early structuralists’ attempts at determining the nature of a phoneme. While the psychological school viewed phonemes 47 as abstract concepts; ‘ideal sounds;’ ‘mental equivalents of a speech sound’ and ‘percepts,’ Bloomfieldians considered phonemes as physical constructs of speech sound- ‘ overt aspects of physical speech event.’ Bloomfield’s perspective dominated this particular linguistic era. In his widely cited definition of the phoneme as ‘a minimal unit of distinctive sound’ (Language: 1933: 79), Bloomfield, as Stephen Anderson observes, implies that a phoneme has a phonetic identity composed of distinctive and non-distinctive physical (phonetic) prOperties. The first mentioned properties, Bloomfield contented, belong to the phoneme while the latter properties, though present in actual realization of a phoneme, are not significant for the linguistic study of speech. Phoneme features are present in sound waves and have linguistic significance in that speakers are trained to produce and respond to these features while ignoring the rest of gross acoustic mass reaching the ear (1933: 79) Bloomfield’s analysis lacked a method that would unequivocally recover phonemic representations (distinctive properties) from phonetic data as defined. In the 19405, while linguists seemed to have reached a consensus on the definition of a phoneme, there were still disagreements on methods of identifying them, Anderson reports. Consequently, more linguists concentrated on formulating methods of extracting phonemes from language. Morris Swadesh’s formulation of basic principles of phonemic analysis (the inductive procedure) is widely cited as the groundbreaking analytical procedure whose refinement was the crux of subsequent statements on phonemic analysis in the post-Bloomfieldian era. The following two principles were central to post- Bloomfieldians’ analysis of the phoneme. 48 Post—Bloomfieldians made the assignment of phones to phonemes subject to what is now generally referred to as the principle of bi-uniqueness. The principle stipulates that if two words or utterances are pronounced alike, then they must receive the same phonemic description; conversely, two words or utterances that have been given the same phonemic analysis must be pronounced alike. The principle of bi-uniqueness was also held to imply that, if a given phone was assigned to a particular phoneme in one position of occurrence, then it must be assigned to the same phoneme in all its other positions of occurrence; it could not be the allophone of one phoneme in one context and of another phoneme in other contexts. A second important principle of the post- Bloomfieldian approach was its insistence that phonemic analysis should be carried out prior to and independently of grammatical analysis. Neither this principle nor that of bi-uniqueness was at all widely accepted outside the post-Bloomfieldian school, and they have been abandoned by generative phonologists In short, the behaviorist approach of American structuralism contended that phonemic structure could be adduced from information present in overt speech. By the 19503, Anderson further reports: Science in general was becoming more concerned with the extent to which theories taken as a whole had explanatory and predictive power within a given domain, bringing coherence and clarity to it, rather than with the mannner in which individual statements within a theory can be operationally verified. With this turn, much of the philosophical rationale for the specific conceptual foundations of structuralism crumbled. (312) Morris Halle and Noam Chomsky’s generative phonology revolutionalized phonemic analysis by showing the inadequacies of the structuralist school. They rejected the behaviorist approach in favor of a mentalist perspective similar to the one that Sapir 49 advocated. The bi-uniqueness principle, the cornerstone of structuralism, was shown to be counterintuitive. Halle emphasized the centrality of rules in phonological description. Halle’s presentation marked a major shift from a concentration on the properties of phonological representation and their elements to a much greater stress on rules of grammar. Chomsky, Halle and Lukoff (1956) had shown that phonological structure was not independent from grammar as structuralists proposed. Generative phonologists, Kenstowicz (1994) argues, interpret phonetic and phonemic identity as two different conceptualizations or representations for phonological information. On the one hand is surface (phonetic) representations which indicate how sounds are actualized in speech-‘the instructions sent to the vocal apparatus to articulate the sounds and acoustic properties that are isolated in order to decode the speech signal’ (7). Underlying (phonological) representation, on the other hand, contain the more abstract information structures that an individual speaker has stored in the brain after acquiring a language. The two representation are however ‘systematically related by phonological rules’ (7). Interest in establishing an interface between phonetics and phonology has been widespread in recent years. In seeking to identify social and linguistic factors that account for language variation, sociolinguists are increasingly seeking more precise methods of extracting and analyzing phonetic and phonemic data. Such concerns for precision have for instance led to development of computer software (as detailed in the previous chapter) that provide detailed acoustic characteristics of sounds to enable researchers to identify even subtle variations in speech production. Researchers interested in the American Northern Cities Vowel Shift have made great inroads in refining methodology in 50 phonetics. Phonemic analysis continue to attract interest and generate more refinements from interdisciplinary research. Labov (1994) has noted that the Minimal Pair Test (MPT) for the identification of phonemes continues to be a preferred method of analysis. This method of determining whether a single sound difference distinguishes the meanings of two words and classifies as separate phonemes the sounds that are responsible for a difference in meaning in such a minimal pair. In other words, sounds are separate phonemes if they contrast in identical environments; that is, if the choice of one sound over another in a particular environment alters the meaning in a word then the two sounds are different phonemes. Labov argues that the MPT as an empirical method for determining the contrastive status of phonemes in a speech community takes into account that listeners perceive and respond to sounds in categorical terms. The test requires a native speaker of a language variety to determine whether two utterances are ‘the same’ or ‘different.’ The pairs of words are identical except for one element, which is represented as one type of sound in one member of the pair and another type in the other member. This method has been widely used in the extraction of phonemes discussed in this chapter. Psycholinguists have shown evidence that points to the fact that the way in which people speak greatly influences their perception of what is said to them. For example, experiments have shown that speakers are unable (prior to training) to pronounce lax and tense vowels if such a distinction is not realized in their language. It is further reported that such people also have difficulty in hearing the difference between such distinctly defined vowels. The basic premise of this motor theory of speech perception is that the 51 perception of speech is structured in terms of linguistic categories. Such a presumption is made in this study. Language acquisition research among bilingual adults and children has shown that native language vowels are stored as prototypes. During speech perception such prototypes act as magnets that ‘pull’ the non-native sound closer to the native prototype, and as a result, spectral differences between the native and non-native sounds are minimized. This is the central premise of the Native Language Magnet Theory; the guiding theoretical framework of this study 52 Chapter Four: Methodology 4.] Introduction This chapter describes the methods used in the collection and analysis of acoustic and perception data. The study focuses only on the onset (steady state) characteristics of vowels, while at the same time being cognizant of the fact that other phonetic features distinguish the vowels within and across varieties. The first half of this chapter describes the respondents and the procedures of collection and analysis of acoustic data. 4. 2 Collection of Acoustic Data 4. 2. 1 The Production-Sample All respondents were carefully selected based on their country of origin (i.e., sub- Saharan African countries with a long tradition of English language usage), level of education (i.e., university or college students) and their first language (i.e., speakers of Kwa and Bantu languages). Male respondents were initially selected for purposes of our preliminary study, and female respondents were later incorporated into the schema of this broader study. Appropriate respondents for this study were identified by the ‘friend of a friend’ network method, as described in Milroy (1980). Initially, I contacted my friends at three African universities‘ who identified a network of friends that made up our sample. Data was collected from male and female university students in Ghana, Kenya and Zimbabwe. The age range, of these native speakers of Kwa and Bantu languages, was between 19-45 years. (See Appendix A for respondents’ profiles). Speakers of the Akan dialect cluster languages were chosen in Ghana (These include speakers of Fanti, 53 Twi, Ashanti and Ewe languages). Shona and Ndebele speakers in Zimbabwe, and the largest Bantu language groups in Kenya: Gikuyu, Gikamba, Lubukusu, and Ekegusii. 4. 2. 2 Data Collection Speech samples were elicited using the questionnaire method. In the first section of the questionnaire, respondents were asked to answer brief biographical questions that elicited age, gender, residence and language background information. Subsequently, the subjects were requested to read a randomized list of 21 monosyllabic words that contained vowels and diphthongs that are relevant to this study. The list included the following words: bead, here/year, hair, hid, caught, bout, good, hard, sod/nod ,boat/goat, tour, buy/pride, sure, board/saw, boy, mud, bad, bird, mood, bet/bed, hue made/name. The word list was adapted from the lexical items used in previous studies on AfrE (see Figure 1). A reading passage specifically constructed to incorporate lexical items already presented in the wordlist was also presented to the subjects. The procedure allowed us to analyze both casual and formal styles of production, on the stylistic continuum, for each vowel under investigation. Each interview was tape recorded. 4. 3. 0 Data Analysis 4. 3. 1 Acoustic Measurement The collected data was subjected to two kinds of analysis: quantitative and qualitative. The most fundamental data, the vowels contained in the word-list, was extracted and frequencies of the first two formants (F1 and F2) calculated using a computer program (SIGNALYZE version 3.12) designed for acoustic analysis. The extraction procedure involved digitizing sound by way of sound input from tape to 1 Collection of data in Africa was conducted by Dr. Harry Akussah of University of Ghana, Dr. Sawasawa Kambewa of Malawi, Maina Mutonya of University of Witwatersrand, S.A.and Dr. 54 computer. Consequently, each word is saved as an acoustic waveform on a Signalyze screen, from which the relevant vowel is identified, isolated, and F1/F 2 scores calculated. Formant measurements were calculated by locating the cursor at an appropriate point of a waveform, using linear predictive coding (LPC). The F1 frequencies are inversely associated with the height dimension, and the F2 frequencies are associated with the front-back dimension. A data file for each respondent was subsequently prepared showing F 1, F2 scores, vowel class, stress and word (see Appendix B). These scores were subsequently loaded into a computer program (PLOTNIK (version 04)), a vowel system analysis program developed by William Labov for plotting vowel systems from formant measurements (see Appendix C 1). The data is recoded, a procedure that reads the orthography of the word and adds phonetic information of the vowel, indicating, in code form, the manner, place of articulation, and voicing of preceding segments and following sequences. A preliminary accuracy evaluation task was performed after tokens were plotted on a PLOTNIK vowel chart. When formant values of the same vowel and subject showed wide variation, we double-checked them by listening to and comparing the spectrograms of the tokens. In cases where a respondent clearly misread a word (e.g. read the word boat as boot, made as mad, or sod as sad) on the word list, the correct word was extracted from the reading passage and measurements of the correctly read vowel replaced the misread one. After data from each group and region is recoded, a group mean is calculated, and all files are normalized. Labov (1998223) explains the need to normalize data: Abraham Ndungu of Kenyatta University, Kenya. 55 [Normalization transforms] all measurements into a single reference grid which will show vowels which sound the same with the same formant values. Ideally, this would duplicate the normalizing process of the human ear and neural network...it has proven to be more successful than others in eliminating effects due to differences in vocal tract length, while preserving those social differences of age and sex that are inherent characteristics of the speech community . Having reduced linguistically irrelevant differences in the production of speech signals through normalization, the resultant acoustic vowel chart becomes an accurate representation of the linguistic aspects of the vowels, facilitating both across-speaker and across-language comparison as B.G. Yang, (1996) demonstrates in his comparative study of American English and Korean vowels. 4. 4 Vowel Perception Data Two perception tests were conducted to determinate listeners ability to identify and discriminate RP vowels: The minimal pair test (MPT) and the identification test (IT). 4. 4. 1 Data Collection Methods Data was collected using a tape recorded stimulus voice and questionnaire as the main data elicitation tools. A minimal pair test (MPT) and identification test (IT) were carefully designed to elicit perceptual responses of lexical items (and vowels indirectly) used in the acoustic analysis presented in the previous chapter (see questionnaire and minimal pair list in Appendix D). In the MPT, respondents were requested to mark whether the each of the fifty-seven pairs of English words recorded on tape were the same words or not. The IT entailed listening to the same fifty -seven pairs and writing on the questionnaire provided what each word was. The minimal pair list was adapted from a randomized list of 21 monosyllables used earlier in the elicitation of acoustic data (see 3.2.2). However, in this case other monosyllabic words were carefully added in order to create minimal pairs, while in some 56 instances words were simply reduplicated to test for a particular vowel. The list included the following pairs: beat/bit, weed/wed, bid/bird, hid/hid, wade/word, bit/bet, meet/meet, mad/mud, hay/hair, bid/beard, made/made, he/hue, bed/bad, bay/beer, bet/bet, bird/bird, beat/bait, don/dawn, bird/bad, bird/bud, wade/wed, bed/bird, bat/bat, had/hard, heard/hoed, bait/bat, bird/board, dam/dime, bat/bout, rug/rug, mud/mad, cat/cot, hut/hurt, gut/got, mud/mode, land/lend, bud/board, got/goat, wooed/wood, law/lure, sod/soy, call/call, but/bought, hot/hot, bought/bout, call/coil, walk/work, boot/boat, caught/cot, cod/cured, cook/coke, good/good, got/gout, he/here/book/book, who/hue, cot/caught. An RP speaker carefully read the list for recording. All respondents listened to the same list read by the same speaker. Subsequently, a correlation of acoustic and perception results was done to determine the vowel systems. 4. 4. 2 The Perception-Sample Responses were elicited from one hundred and sixty students (undergraduate and graduate) from universities in Ghana, Malawi and Kenya. The demographic characteristics of this perception-sample is similar to the production-sample discussed above. In a nutshell, the age range of the sample is 19-45, both male and female respondents participated, Kwa and Bantu speakers were identified to participate in the survey. Respondents were identified using Leslie Milroy’s friend of friend network method. 57 The RP speaker who read the stimulus words is a thirty year old graduate student at Michigan State University. By the time of the recording, he had lived in the United States for less than two years. Acoustic and perception data is discussed in the following chapter. 58 Chapter 5: Data Analysis 5. 1 Data Presentation and Discussion In this section, vowel production among male and female speakers of AfrE is analyzed using Si gnalyze and Plotnik computer software. As stated above, the current study focuses on vowel production among educated male and female members of Kwa (in Ghana) and Bantu (in Kenya and Zimbabwe) language groups, who have acquired English as a second or third language. This narrowed focus takes cognizance of social and linguistic factors that are bound to affect a non-native speaker’s production and perception of foreign language phones (i.e. English). English in sub-Saharan Africa has varied degrees of contact with diverse indigenous African languages in different language settings. In many settings, dominant indigenous languages compete strongly with English in societal perception and assignment of roles. Apart from the dense multlingualism of the speech communities in question, our study takes account of the diversity of multilingual backgrounds among individual AfrE speakers and its assumed effect on production and perception. As postulated in our guiding theoretical assumption, the NLM theory (see chapter 3), non- native speakers will perceive, and presumably produce, foreign vowels in terms of native phonological categories and as exemplars of native vowel types. Considering further that many sub-Saharan countries have a diversity of L1 speakers (Nilotic, Semitic, Cushitic, Bantu, Kwa and others), it was necessary to seek a homogeneous sample. Earlier studies seemed to overlook this linguistic diversity. 59 The following analysis and discussion is based on acoustic measurements of the physical qualities of vowels. In the next section, the non-physical qualities of the vowels are measured using the vowel perception tests. AfrE vowel variation is illustrated in Schmied’s comparison of RP vowel realization with those of WAfrE and EAfrE speakers (Figure l). The illustration shows how the RP thirteen-vowel system is realized as a seven-vowel system among WAfrE speakers and a five-vowel system among EAfrE speakers through such phonological processes such as mergers, lowering and fronting. The reduction in the AfrE vowel systems may be a clear testimony of the influence of phonological systems of indigenous African languages in the production of RP vowels by AfrE speakers. Such a claim would reflect the primary tenet of the Native Language Magnet Theory (see Chapter 3) According to earlier studies in the field, the WAfrE vowel system contrasts lax and tense front and back central vowels /e/, /8/ and /o/, lo /, while EAfrE does not. Both systems merge their lax and tense high vowels /i/, /I/ and /u/, /U/. 5. 1. l Schmied's Analysis of AfrE Vowel Systems Figures 7, 8, and 8 recreate RP and AfrE vowel systems presented in Figure l in a vowel chart configuration, highlighting suggested phonological processes that occur in the production of AfrE vowels. Lowering, backing and fronting, are the major processes that result from decentralization of RP central vowels. Since F 1 and F2 scores are generally unavailable in the literature, Figures 7, 8, and 9 are not acoustic representations of the vowel systems, but extracts intended to display relations among the vowel systems as represented in Figure 1. Therefore, unlike 60 F2 n 0001 . i bead I Did UDUII e bade 3 bird 0 bode 8 bed Abud 9 board 1 a bed a bard Figure 7 - representation of the RP vowel system as illustrated in Figure 1 our later vowel charts, which are based on instrumental measurements of F1 and F2 scores of each vowel nucleus, such scores are not indicated. At this juncture, we should note that throughout this study vowel charts are presented in a two-formant illustration , irrespective of whether the presentation is based on approximations (Figures 7, 8, 9) or precise instrumental measurements of vowel nuclei. First formant frequencies (F1) are plotted on the ordinate (vertical axis), indicating the height of the tongue in the oral cavity. Second formant frequencies (F 2) are plotted on the abscissa (horizontal axis) indicating the position of the vowel along the front/back dimension of the oral cavity. Figure 7 shows the configuration of an RP vowel system. On the front/back (F2) dimension, three types of vowels are identified: front vowels [ i, I, e, e, a], central [3, /\ ], 61 and back vowels [ u:, u, D, o, a ]. On the height dimension (Fl), RP vowels are distinguished as high [ i: I, u, 0], mid [ e, 8, 3, A, o, D] or low [ a, a ]. A tense/lax F 2 band . u ' 010 ' B “9””. 94. "LEI [El ‘ bad Figure 8 - A representation of WAfrE as illustrated in Figure l. (close/open) distinction is evident in the vowels [e/ 8] [ill], [u:/ u] [b /o]. Moreover, RP has a vowel length distinction in [ i:/I, u:/ u, and D /o ]. In the discussion to follow, it is necessary to consider the arrangement of the phonological space among AfrE speakers, particularly in regard to RP's central vowels, vowel length, and tense/lax distinction. In the WAfrE seven-vowel system that Schmied suggests (Figure 8), RP central vowels [3 and A] are backed to [D]. [a] is lowered to [D]. The tense/lax distinction is maintained in the front mid vowels [e/e]. 62 Figure 9 illustrates Schmied’s EAfrE five-vowel system which (like WAfrE) has no central vowels. RP's central vowels [3 and A] are lowered to [a]. In the discussion to follow, our dissension with Schmied on this issue is not in the characterization of the EAfrE system, but in the assumption that the EAfrE and SAfrE vowel systems are F 2 1 p001 Dead 11 1 bid pull l bade f . bode e bed ’5 I; board 0 bird bud bad bard Figure 9 - A representation of EAfrE as illustrated in Figure 1 identical. RP's tense/lax distinction, evident in the WAfrE mid vowel system above, is lost in the mergers of high, mid, and low vowels in EAfrE, and, by inference, SAfrE. Schmied’s discussion of diphthongs is limited to the following observation that other studies have echoed throughout the literature: Diphthongs tend to have only marginal status and to be monophthongized. This is certainly true for the closing diphthongs /ei/ and /'u /, where the second element is hardly heard in many African varieties, thus almost coinciding with the /e/ and /o/ phonemes. Diphthongs with a longer glide are preserved, but they are not really pronounced as falling diphthongs, i.e., with less emphasis on the second element than the first, but rather as double monophthongs (e.g. /oi/, /au/). All the centering diphthongs (/i'/ , /e'/, /u'/ tend to be pronounced as opening diphthongs or double monophthongs (/ia, ea, ua/) (Schmied, 1991:59). 63 In the following subsections, we compare our findings with Schmied's and, by implication, the vast majority of others who have commented on AfrE vowel systems. 5. 2 An Empirical Analysis of AfrE vowel systems In this section, we present and discuss vowel production among respondents from Kenya, Zimbabwe and Ghana, based on the results derived from Signalyze and Plotnik analysis. The results for each region are presented in three phases. First, an overview of vowel production by all respondents in each region is presented by combining all data files into one. The file is then analyzed using Plotnik, resulting into vowel charts in Figures 10, 11, and 12. Secondly, each general vowel plot is followed by individual sample charts that exemplify the general trend in the majority of cases among male and female respondents. Finally, observations are made and conclusions drawn. It is important to ma at this juncture that the plot for “all tokens” in each region is supposed to confirm the general picture and to justify the token groupings made in the illustrative individual plots. For the instance, the token groupings shown in the individual plots in each region and gender, is based on the emerging clustering of tokens when all individual vowel plots in each region and gender are considered. The two individual plots were chosen as the exemplars of the entire group. All F1/F2 scores are shown in Appendix B2. 5. 2. l Kenyan Speakers 64 F2 3000 2900 2600 2400 2200 2030 I80) I 600 I 400 12(1) I 000 300 200 l l l l l l L l L l l F V f " hid m“ bead "W good i gear . mood } 400a ‘ —- ‘ . F——_ ‘ hai r sure a 500. e made bet I 50d board name ' 93" caught L - J 6004 fi * f 4] [E] II bad pride bout 7”“ bad bird hard - mud aoo- bl rd ‘1? ‘ J k 900- All Kenyans (20 IMP) Figure 10 - Vowel Plot of all Kenyan Respondents Figure 10I represents the overall vowel production of 20 male and female university students from Kenya. Based on the evidence presented, the vowel system is evidently a peripheral five-vowel system that lacks central vowels. Vowel clustering in Figure 10 can be summarized as follows: 65 /i/ bead,[hid, year] /e/ made, bet, hair, name, [hid, year] /a/ bad, bird , hard, pride, bout, mud /o/ sod, goat , board, caught, boy, sure. /u/ good, mood Considering that such general plots may obscure finer details in the vowel production, this study analyzes each vowel production in each individual system, and assumes that patterns recurring in a majority of cases represent vowel production of the entire sample. 5. 2. 2 Kenyan Male Respondents The following two normalized vowel charts are representative of the vowel production among eleven Kenyan male respondents. The collection, analysis and acoustic measurements of the vowels is explained above. The normalized F1/F2 scores of all respondents are shown in Appendix C2. Figure 10.1 illustrates a plot of the F1 and F2 frequencies for the 22 tokens produced by Njenga-a 24 year old Kenyan male university student. Njenga’s vowel system, like all other AfrE vowel systems analyzed in this study, is a peripheral system that, unlike the RP system, contains no central vowels. Central vowels are fronted, 1 Figures 10, 11 and 12 were carefully edited in order to reduce dense clustering of the 400 tokens and labels that would otherwise have obscured the details in the chart. 66 FZ-FI 3000 2800 2600 2400 2200 2000 1800 1600 1400 1200 1000 800 200 I I l L l l I l l I l 300 - 400- sod fopen 500] ' 0 J 9M 0 board 'b gang“ boy 600-1 E [:Z] Ilbad (bout 7001 1"” d pride 1| bud 800- Emmi E 900- 1000- | Nl'engaglwormawralgsenza | Figure 10.1 - Njenga’s Vowel Plot backed or even lowered to merge with non central vowels. In Figure 10.1, vowels contained in the tokens bead and hid are realized as [i]; vowels in the words made and bet as [6]; year, hair, and name , are produced as [8]; bad, hard, pride, bout, bird and mud [A] are pronounced as [a]; sad, open, goat, caught, board and boy as [0]; while 67 FZ-Fl 3000 28100 26100 2100 22100 2000 18100 isoo 1100 nice 1000 900 200 300-1 [ 'bb’“ I ] d‘mood u Ohid 09m 400- [ bmade CObeiJ 05m caught 0 97'0”? 500- [ 9w onair board '5‘; t 0 go: ‘0 name 8 6004 . ED Iblrd I]? pride Brim a W“ v‘ 700. bout 300. 900- 1000- | Nl'ogugwlformallvrbanéenya | Figure 10.2 - Njogu’s Vowel system ( a Kenyan Male) mood , good and sure are realized as [u]. Njogu’s vowel production (Figure 10.2) is almost identical to Njenga’s (Figure 10.1).The only discernible difference is that while Njenga realizes the vowel in sure as [u], Njogu realizes it as [0]. Few other Kenyan male respondents realize the vowel in question as [o]. 68 Based on acoustic data analyzed from our Kenya male sample (as represented in the two vowel plots above), AfrE vowel production among Kenyan males may be summarized in the representation shown below. Tokens in parentheses indicate across- speaker variations identified in the plots. For instance, parenthesized [hair, bet] indicate that there exist a difference in the respondents pronunciation of vowels in those tokens, such that some respondents will realize vowels contained in these words in an identical manner to other vowels of the cluster that they belong to. For example [hair and bet] are pronounced as either /e/ or lal. /i/ bead, hid /e/ made, [bet, hair] /8/ year, name, [hair, bet] /a/ bad, bird , hard, pride, bout, mud /0/ so, open, goat , board, caught, boy, [sure] /u/ good, mood, [sure] 5. 2. 3 Kenyan Female Respondents Now let us examine the vowel production among educated female respondents from Kenya. Figures 10.3 and 10. 4 are representative of other individual vowel charts of this homogenous sample of 11 respondents. The variation in the production of [on] as in the word boat is the only distinct difference in production among these speakers. Based on acoustic measurements of F1 and F2 frequencies, Mukami (Figure 10.3) realizes the English vowels under investigation as follows: Vowels contained in the tokens bead and hid, hue and here are realized as [i]; made, hair, bed are produced as 69 [e]; hard, bird ,bad, buy , mud, and bout are realized as [a]; while saw, nod, boy, and boat are pronounced as [o] and good, mood, tour, and sure are produced as [u]. Note, however, how close some of the “0” (or mid-back) tokens are to the “low” set (e.g. nod, hard). 3000 2800 2500 2400 2200 2000 1800 1600 1400 1200 1000 800 t 200 I I g I I I l l I I I 300- . $0?! 01110 good 0 here i 'bbead ll 0 mood 400. d‘hue 1 0 0" our ' . l 500. “a" 0 'Rmade e bed 600- [El 700- 300+ 9004 1000- [ Mukami,21,i,'ionnal,0rban,xenya J Figure 10.3 - Mukami’s Vowel System ( a Kenyan female) 7f) Kamba’s vowel system (Figure 10.4) is almost identical to Mukami’s (Figure 10.3) except for the production of [au] as in bout, which in Kamba’s system is realized as [o]. Kamba’s low vowels are also more compact (in a back position). F2 - i1 2003000 2800 2600 2400 2200 2000 1800 1600 1400 1200 1000 800 300~ ' Ohid ' I 11 head mood sun I he re I 400‘ l hue goodo our 8W boat 500« I l bout 0 mode bed 00],] 600- hair e W [E 5" b . I bod “U 700. El bird 1- a hard H 800 Kamba,30,F,Urban,Kenga Figure 10.4 - Kamba’s Vowel System ( a Kenyan Female) Acoustic analysis of vowel production among educated female respondents in Kenya can be summarized as follows: 71 /i/ hue, bead, hid, here /e/ mad, hair, bed /a/ hard, bird , bad, buy , mud, bout /0/ saw, nod , boy, boat, bout /u/ good, mood, tour , sure An analysis of both male and female respondents from Kenya shows a clear pattern of vowel clustering which varies only in the respondents’ realization of the front mid vowel as both tense and lax. It should be noted that only a few of the respondents do not make this distinction. The only other significant variation is that while male respondents realize [Is] as /e/, female speakers produce it as /i/. As might be expected, there is more range to the targets in low-vowel territory. The gender distinction noted here is worth of serious investigation in future studies. Previous studies have lumped the speaker together and no gender distinctions have been reported. 5. 3 Zimbabwe Respondents Figure 1 1 represents an edited plot of F l and f2 frequencies for over 400 tokens produced by 20 respondents in Zimbabwe. Like the EAfrE vowel chart in Figure 10, this is also a peripheral system with no central vowels (although it can be argued that /8/ is fairly central relative EAfrE and WAfrE systems). 72 F2 3000 28.00 26100 2100 2290 2000 18100 1600 1400 1200 100) 800 200 f ' V hue 300a U mood J0. 900d 400. tour 1 - l ‘ sov sure board 500‘ '0 mode 0 v nod 0" bed name 9001 0’ boot , bed 600- b1rdD D 3 bad D l I E ‘ D 1 mod bill] 700‘ bout ‘ mud pride 800~ '0 :31 hard 11 ’00 h .1000 ‘ A All Zimbabweans (20 M&F) Figure l 1 - A vowel plot for all Zimbabwean Respondents However, unlike EAfrE, this is a six-vowel system. The results can be summarized as follows: /i/ bead, hid, here, [hue] /e/ hair, [bird, name, made, bed] /8/ bad,[bird, name, made, bed] /a/ hard, pride, bout, mud /o/ sod, nod , goat, boa /u/ good, mood, tour, [hue] 73 5. 3. l Zimbabwean Male Respondents 3000 2000 26100 2490 2200 2000 1000 16100 1100 1200 1000 000 200 ‘ fl mm _me l 0 good 400- hid 0 mod 0‘ 6 09h gear 1* 500_ 0" Sure % 001] sod goat 0 € board 6004 :00 caught E 700- Amud '17 ' a pride 800- Jard + I Jones,33,F0rm01,Rurai,Zimbobwe Figure l 1.1 - Jones’ Vowel System (a Zimbabwean Male) Figure 11.1 illustrates a plot of the F l and F2 frequencies for the 22 tokens produced by Jones-a 35 year old Zimbabwean male university student. In this chart , vowels contained in the tokens bead and hid are realized as [i]; year and hair as [e]; 74 made, bet, name, bad , bird as [8]; hard, pride, bout, and mud as [a]; sod, open, goat, caught, board, sure, and boy as [0]; while mood and good are realized as [u]. Ndanga’s vowel systems (Figure 11.2) shows minor variations from Jones’ (Figure 1 1.1). Inversely, Ndanga realizes the onset of the diphthong in name as [e], and the vowel onset in hair as [8], unlike Jones who realize them as [a] and[e] respectively. Furthermore, Ndanga realizes the vowel in caught as [a], while the other Jones realizes it as [0]. These variations are evident among other respondents in our Zimbabwe sample. Zimbabwean male respondents English vowel production may be summed up as follows: . /i/ bead , hid /e/ year, name, hair /8/ made, bed, bad, bird /a/ hard, pride, bout, mud, [caught] /0/ sod, open , goat, board, boy, sure, [caught] /u/ good, mood 75 3000 28100 26100 2100 2200 2000 18100 16100 1100 1200 1000 800 200 300 7 u moo 400 1 0 9°“ hot; 500 - so sod ‘7' goat V obet bird “board 6 hair sure ’7' open 0 600 8 ‘ ‘0 made I bad [E L f M" Vcauqht . ard 70°“ fiyprid'va mud a 800 - 900- 1000- ] Ndang alarmaIIUrban. Zimbabwe | Figure l 1.2 - Ndanga’s Vowel System (a Zimbabwean Male) 5. 3. 2 Zimbabwean Female Respondents Turning our attention to vowel production among educated AfrE female speakers from Zimbabwe, another peripheral vowel system is evident. Figures 11.3 and 11.4 are representative charts of all individual speakers in a sample of eleven speakers. 76 3000 28100 26100 2100 2200 2000 18100 16100 1100 1200 1000 800 200 300- 0 good O’hue “no 0" 0 tour II 400-- mood . bou . hair made '2) 50° 6° e bird saw eh“ nod . 0 500. had I I hard +'Ind 700 - mud 'wa soo- 900~ 1000‘ l Kapindarfilfiormallwbanthnbabwe I Figure l 1.3 - Kapinda’s Vowel System ( a Zimbabwean Female) Kapinda’s normalized F1 and F2 scores are presented in Figure 11.3. Like all other Kapinda realizes the English vowels presented in word list form as follows: Vowels contained in the tokens bead and hid and here are realized as [i]; made, hair, bed and bird are produced as [e], although bird seems fairly central; hard, bad, buy, mud, and bout are realized as [a]; while saw, nod, boy, and boat are pronounced as [o]; and good, mood, tour, sure, and hue are produced as [u]. 77 Tamika’s (Figure 11.4) vowel production is similar to Kapinda’s (Fig. 11.3) except for bead which is realized as [e], and sure and tour, which are produced as [0]. Most female respondents realize tour as [0]. Again, bird is fairly central. 300 — 400- 500 — 600 — 700 - 800 - 900 - 1000—t Figure “SC 00 28100 26100 24:00 2200 2000 18.00 16100 1‘100 1200 1000 800 h . 11 9°“ 0 mood re . hid l “"9 0’ 0" ma 9 e % hair e El bird sunI 9 'b bead 0 9 tour boy 2)“ ‘I boat em «I I fig bzd (NM 1.0:: a A mud l Tamika,32,r,rormaI,UrbanTzimbabwe ] l 1.4 - Tamika’s Vowel System ( a Zimbabwean Female) 78 Vowel production among Zimbabwean female respondents may be summarized as follows: /i/ hid, here,[ bead] /e/ made, hair, bed, bird, [bead] /a/ hard, bad, buy , mud, bout /0/ saw, nod , boy, boat, tour ,sure /u/ hue, good, mood,[sure, tour A gender distinction similar to the one already identified in discussion of the Kenyan sample is also evident among the Zimbabwean AfrE speakers. Male respondents have a lax and tense distinction for the front mid vowel while female speakers do not. Moreover, while male respondents front RP’s central vowel [3] as in bird to [8], and raise the low back vowel [a] to [8], female respondents barely front [3] as in bird to [8], but they do not raise [a]. It is worth emphasizing that gender distinction is not confined to the Kenyan sample as the Zimbabwean data shows. Further research into the variation should illuminate more characteristics of vowel variance within and across English varieties spoken in sub-Saharan Africa. It is worth mentioning at this juncture that the fronting of the central vowel [3] to [8] seems to be the major variation between Bantu speakers of Eastern Africa (Kenya) and Southern Africa (Zimbabwe). Moreover, [us] as in tour and sure is consistently realized as [u] by Kenyan respondents, but some Zimbabweans realize the monophthongized diphthong as either [0] or [u]. This finding casts doubt on earlier claims that AfrE speakers consistently produced [09] as [u]. 79 5. 4 Ghanaian Respondents Figure 12 represents vowel frequencies of all the token produced by 20 Ghanaian university students. This five-vowel system is peripheral and contains no central vowels. F2 2003000 2W0 16100 2000 2200 2000 1§30 |§00 14100 12(1) 1900 89) r , ‘ 30°. hue ll 0 0’ mood ‘o bead J made good 400- gear 0 good i ' 9 here 6 sure boat 5004 L o hid J ' 9 board bog a saw bird ‘9 b d ‘ but 0 e 600- II ca ht [E] bird “9 El 9 0 pride 700- k 9hair J i; 'b D - bout (i' buy 300- bad El 900- ‘ mud '3' hard ‘9 J L l llll Ghanaians (20 M81?) | Figure 12- Vowel Plot of All Ghanaian Respondents The results may be summarized as follows: /i/ hue, made, bead , hid , here /e/ hair, bird, bed /a/ hard, bad, buy, mud, [bout], pride 80 /0/ saw, nod, boy, tour, [boat], caught /u/ good, mood, sure, [boat],[ bout] 5. 4. 1 Ghanaian Male Respondents Figure 12.] illustrates a plot of the F 1 and F2 frequencies for the 22 tokens produced by Kwame , a 27 year old male university student from Ghana. In this chart, IE 3t00 2930 2600 2400 2200 200) 1800 1600 1400 1200 1000 800 200 J l 1 l l 1 l 1 1 l 1 300- . bead ll 09°“ Ill) ohtd d‘mood 400- goat open Sure 500- 600- 700- 800- Kasem,27,M,Formal,Urban,Ghana Figure 12.1 — Kwame Vowel System ( a Ghanaian Male) 81 vowels contained in the tokens bead and hid are realized as [i]; year and name 3C00 2800 2600 2400 2200 2000 1800 1600 1400 1200 1000 800 200 J l l l l l l 1 l l l E 300 - . { I 0 bid good u bead R0 0 r? mood 400— open 0 ‘gbo 500 - , * gear 9 ha" 0 E bird bet 8 600- 700 - 800- 900- 1000- | Twi,42,M,Formal,Urhan,Ghana | Figure 12.2 - Kojo’s Vowel System ( a Ghanaian Male) 82 as [e]; and hair,, bet, made and bird as [8]; hard, pride, bout, bad and mud as [a]; sod, caught and board are pronounced as [0]; open, goat, sure, and boy are realized as [0]; while mood and good are realized as [u]. In comparison to Kwame’s vowel production (Figure 12.1), Kojo’s system (Figure 12.2) represents other individual systems in, the sample that vary from those that resemble Kasem’s in their inverse realizations of made and year as either [e] or [8], and goat and board as [0] or [o]. This calls into question, of course, the idea of a seven (rather than five) vowel system. The clustering of [i, u, a] is also identical in a majority of cases. AfrE vowel production among educated male Ghanaians may be summarized as follows: /i/ bead, hid /e/ name, [year, made] /8/ hair, bet, bird, [year, made] /a/ hard, bad, pride, bout, mud /o/ caught, sod , [board, goat] /0/ open , boy, sure, [board, goat] /u/ good, mood 5. 4. 2 Ghanaian Female Respondents Let us examine AfrE vowel production among educated Ghanaian women, based on a controlled sample of eleven Kwa language speakers. Figures 12.3 and 12.4 were identified as representative Ghanaian speakers. 83 200 3000 28100 26100 2100 2200 2000 1000 MPG 1100 1200 1000 800 300 - 400- 500 - 600 - 700 - 800 - 900 - 1000- ’ 1 0 good ll ‘7' boat 0 sure mood L O, 1 nod tour saw r V a bout v M I bad+ hard A mud x 1 I Twi,42,F,Formal,Urban,5hana j Figure 12.3 - Akosua’s Vowel System ( a Ghanaian Female) Akosua (Figure 12.3) realizes the English vowels presented in tokens shown in the chart as follows: Vowels contained in the tokens hue and here are realized as [i]; bead , hid and made are produced as [e]; hair, bed and bird are realized as [a]; hard, bad. buy, mud, and bout are realized as [a]; while saw, nod, boy and tour are pronounced as [0], while vowels in good, mood, sure and boat are produced as [u]. 84 In contrast to Akosua (Figure 12.3) Efua (Figure 12.4) produces vowel in hid and head as /i/, bed as /e/ and boat as [o]. The differences across female speakers seem to be in vowel height in such tokens as hid, bead, and here. There seem to be differences in the tense/lax distinction of the vowel in bed. Some respondents pronounceboat with an n3C00 2800 26'00 2400 2200 2000 1800 16IOO 14100 1200 1000 800 20.. 300 - 400 - 500 — 600 - 700 - 800 - {' bout 07 bug + hard 900 -‘ I had A mud 1000— | Wasa;36,FnFormaIIUrbanIGhana | Figure 12.4 - Efua’s Vowel System ( a Ghanaian Female) [0] and some with an [u]. Such variations merit further detailed acoustic measurements. A summary of Ghanaian female vowel production contained in charts discussed above is as follows: 85 /i/ hue, [bead , hid , here] lei made, [bead, hid, here], [bed] /8/ hair, bird, [bed] /a/ hard, bad, buy, mud, [bout] /0/ saw, nod, boy, tour, [boat] /u/ good, mood, sure, [boat], [bout] An overview of Ghanaian sample shows that both male and female vowels systems have a lax and tense front middle vowel, front RP’s central vowel [3] as in bird to [a], but differ in the realization of [ua] as in sure. While male respondents realize the monophthongized diphthong as [0], females pronounce it as [u]. 5. 5 A Comparative Analysis of Acoustic Results The primary aim of this study is to verify conclusions drawn in previous studies pertaining to variation in AfrE vowel systems. Conclusions drawn in the current study are based on empirical analysis of data collected from a relatively homogeneous population of well-educated male and female AfrE speakers from Ghana, Kenya and Zimbabwe. Our Ghanaian sample is made up of native speakers of Kwa languages, while Kenyan and Zimbabwean AfrE speakers are native speakers of Bantu languages. All respondents speak English as their second or even third language. Let us compare results of our acoustic analyses presented in this chapter with some primary results documented in earlier studies in the field. The results are discussed in more detail in the literature review section ( 1.4.0.). 86 Schmied (1991) attributes AfrE variation to the ‘deviation’ in the production of the RP long central vowel [3] (as in bird) by speakers of each regional speech community. He concludes that [3] backs to [o] in WAfrE, fronts and lowers to [a] or fronts to [e] in EAfrE, while in SAfrE it fronts to [e]. The two possibilities in EAfiE are not elaborated. This variation is presented in Schmied's illustration as shown in Figure 1. This primary hypothesis, which is widely echoed in the literature, is not corroborated in the evidence from the acoustic analysis presented in this study. Ghanaian and Zimbabwean speakers front the RP central vowel /3/ to/ 8/ while Kenyans lower it to /a/. The central vowel //\l as in bud is lowered by Kenyans and Ghanaians speakers while Zimbabweans front it to /e/. Furthermore, evidence from our male and female respondents from Ghana clearly shows that /I\/ is realized as [a] and not as [o], as Todd (1984) claims (see 2. 2. 2. 1). Moreover, other previous claims not supported by our evidence from acoustic analyses include Bokamba’s (1992:21) observation that //\/ as in bud is produced as [o], and [3] as inbird is realized as [0] and Schmied’s (1991259) claim that all the centring diphthongs (lial, /ee/, lua/ tend to be pronounced as opening diphthongs (lia, ea, ual). Our analysis, however, shows variation in realization of lia/ and /ea/ as either /i/ or /e/ and /ua/ as /0/ or /u/, while /au/ is realized as /a/ or /o/. These variations occur in all three varieties. 87 5. 6. l Kenyans Acoustic analyses of Kenyans English vowel production generally support previous characterizations of the EAfrE vowel system as shown in Figures 1 and 4. However, contrary to earlier claims, acoustic evidence presented above shows that some male respondents make a tense and lax distinction in the front mid vowel area. 5. 6. 2 Ghanaians Acoustic analysis of vowel production among our Ghanaian respondents does not concur with Todd’s claim that [A] backs to [o ] in WAfrE . Trudgill and Hannah’s (1985) illustration (Figure 5), makes identical claims to Schmied’s (Figure 1), and therefore disagrees with ours in the same way. Sey (1973) compares RP and educated Ghanaian English (Figure 4) and claims that the vowel /3/ as in bird is fronted and realized as /e/ contrary to Schmied's claims. Sey claims that the vowel /A/ as in the word bud is realised as either /0/ or /8/. The significance of Sey’s observations to this study lies in the fact that it is the only study we know of in the previous literature that claims that RP’s central vowel /3/ is fronted to [8] by WAfrE speakers. All other studies claim that the vowel is backed to [0]. Our analysis concurs with Sey’s observation. However, like other studies mentioned in this study, Sey claims that educated Ghanaians realize /A/ as /0/. Sey goes a step further and notes that /A/ is realized as /0/ or /8/. Empirical evidence from a diverse population 88 of male and female Kwa language speakers at the University of Ghana unequivocally shows, however, that /l\/ is realized as [ 3]. Based on our analysis, it is clear that our sample of educated Ghanaian speakers does not realize the vowel in bird as [0], but rather as [8]. Moreover, the RP central vowel /A/ as in the word mud is not realized as [0], but as [a]. Therefore, that broad generalization from earlier research regarding the nature of WAfrE vowel system does not hold, particularly among our Ghanaian male respondents 5. 6. 3 Zimbabweans Based on our analysis, Zimbabwean speakers do not make a vowel quality contrast; the only discernible lax/tense distinctions is among male respondents who make a distinction between lax and tense front mid vowel. The vowels in the words bet/bird/bad/made are realized as /e/. The vowels in hard/mud are realized as [a], while those in board/sod/caught are realized as [o]. Suffice it to say, the phonological systems proposed in earlier studies fairly represent the phonetic reality, but the problem lies in grouping. In conclusion, the strength and significance of our findings lies in the evidence we presented to contradict major claims presented in primary studies in the field. Generalizations about the realignment of RP’s central vowels by diverse multilingual AfrE speakers residing in broadly defined linguistic boundaries may be true in some cases, but certainly not wholly representative of the homogeneous samples that we analyzed. 89 This section has focused on variation in the production of AfrE vowels. Although empirical methods of analysis have carefully been applied to ensure a precise presentation of data, our study will be more meaningful if we correlate acoustic cues with phonemic representations through perception tests. As Lieberman and Blumstein (1993: 170) argues: Although it is possible to perform precise analyses of speech signals using electronic instruments and computer programs that effect various mathematical transformations of signal, these analyses are in themselves meaningless. we can never be certain that we have actually isolated the acoustic cues that people use to transmit information to each other unless we run psychoacoustic studies in which human listeners respond to acoustic signals that differ with respect to the acoustic cues that we think are relevant. This is the task we seek to accomplish in the following section. 5. 7 AfrE Listeners’ Vowel Perception 5. 7. 1 Data Analysis Data from each region was calculated separately in order to determine how each vowel was perceived in the IT and MPT. The former entailed calculation of correct and incorrect identifications of every vowel by each respondent. After accounting for all responses in each region, frequency scores were calculated to establish any trends or patterns of identification. Similarly, MPT analysis sought to calculated and subsequently identify any patterns in the correct and erroneous discrimination of the pairs. Suffice it to say, the focus in the analysis of IT data is the onset (steady state) characteristics of the vowels and not the word. For instance, if a stimulus pair bed/bad was identified as bent/ bird, the first vowel in the pair was marked as correctly identified in spite of the error in matching the word, while the error in identifying the vowel of the 90 second word was counted as such. Although the entire raw data is shown in Appendix E, Table 1 is an example of such vowel perception in each region. The first column contains the pairs of words that listeners heard on the tape, and vowels tested. The other columns show the percentage of respondents who identified the intended vowel, or an alternative one. The former is represented in the first line of each cell, while any additional scores in each cell represent the latter. For example, the first score box in the last column shows that 84 % of listeners from the EAfrE speaking region correctly identified in their questionnaires that the first word of the stimulus pair meat/meat contained the vowel [i]. 63% of those listeners also identified the second word as containing [i]. The second line in the same box shows that 5 % and 21% of EAfrE listeners misidentified the first and second words respectively, as containing [1] instead of [i]. Data in the table should be interpreted in that manner unless otherwise stated. Table 1- Identification Results of Vowel [i] Stimulus Ghanaians Zimbabweans Kenyan respondents minimal pairs respondents respondents perception of stimulus words played to perception of stimulus perception of stimulus vowels (in %) i ~ i i ~ i 94-94 i ~ i 98-100 i ~ i 84—63 meat/meat a ~ 6 06-06 I ~I 05-21 i~I i~I 68-48 i~I 41-24 i~I 75-21 beat/bit I ~ i 32-38 I ~ i 59-41 I ~ i 21- 28 e 26 e 49 i ~ 6 i ~ 6 64-43 i ~ 6 43-59 i ~ 6 57-14 beat/bait 1~A 31-17 I~ai 47-13 a~a 60-60 a 13 91 i ~ 8 i ~ 8 100-22 i ~ 8 98-67 i ~ 8 93-65 weed/wed 3 61 la ~O 02-19 ai ~ ai 07-19 i ~ ia i ~ ia 74-100 i ~ ia 79-76 i ~ ia 68-100 he/here ia l7 ia~i 21-24 ia 32 i ~ ju i ~ ju 85—73 i ~ ju 94-58 i ~ ju 82-69 he/hue ju ~ i 15-17 ia ~ i 06-39 ia ~ i 09-32 Having calculated the frequency scores for each vowel in each pair as shown above, an effort was made to determine the overall perception of each vowel in each region. Towards this end, mean values of identification responses to each vowel were calculated by adding up frequency scores and dividing them by the total number of responses. For instance, to determine the mean value [i], all frequency scores for correct identification and misidentifications are added up separately and divided appropriately. 5. 8 Identification Results and Discussion Two types of charts are presented in the discussion of some vowels or groups of vowels. The bar charts seeks to represent visually respondents’ identification of stimulus vowel in percentages, while the vowel charts reflect the same information on a simulated vowel space. Identification of i e oandu 92 Figure 13.1 shows that over 60% of respondents in each region correctly identified vowels [i], [e], [o], and [u]. It is worth noting the high intelligibility score of 100 goo-“i -3. do; 8 _, __ 80; g sgggs NEE: —. as madam J. 53555: :Nss; \g 285 '§ 3 fii§= ammo] . u \.... \...: III\ 50! I §sgga Kass; EN- oof \ a as: " “11M :2; ii % 50$- oooooo fgiifl be gag Gh Zim Ken Figure 13.1 - AfrE Listeners’ Identification of [i], [o], [u], and [e] middle vowels [e] and [o] in all three regions. Earlier perception studies have concluded that listeners easily identify high vowels [i] and [u], and in many cases the low vowel [a]. For instance, Lieberman and Blumstein’s (1993) physiological theory for vowels (see section 1.3), argue that quantal vowels [i] and [u] are easy to identify because the F1 and F2 of each of these vowels converge to yield well-defined spectral peaks, making them maximally distinct. Furthermore, Bladon and Lindblom (1981) report that experiments in vowel perception have shown that listeners use spectral information to access vowel quality. They argue that listener’s are capable of approximating the length of a speaker’s supralaryngeal vocal based on spectral information, and to consequently normalize the vocal tract within the parameters of an appropriate vowel space. The detailed mechanism of vowel perception 93 perception are not within the purview of this study, but it worth noting the high intelligibility rate of these vowels may point towards a maximal distinctness of these ull 1 bad u p >70°/ >80% 0 e bade a bought >80% >65% Figure 13.1: AfrE Listeners’ Identification of [i], [o], [u], and [e] vowels. I IIIIIIIL' iflTear [ia]— Iboy [ oi] II sure [Ua] 1 fl hue [ju] , . .W mfiuuunuu. i , I f f f I f I f l g I r r r r r r“ iiiiiiiiiiiiihi Figure 13.2 - AfrE Listeners’ Identification of [i], [o], [u], and [e] 94 However, this study shows middle vowels [e] and [o], and not the lower vowel [a], as the other easily identified vowel, besides the documented [i] and [11]. Moreover, Figure 14 - AfrE Listeners’ Identification of [ia], [oi], [ua] and [ju] Over 50% of listeners correctly identified diphthongs containing vowels [ i], [ o] , and [u] as Figure 14 shows. The data here, seems to corroborate earlier evidence in Figure 13.1 that the vowels [i], [o], and [u] are easily identified for AfrE listeners; but not [e 22:3 B’bad [a] {liltzllilllbliliiElIE a bud[A] Figure 15.1 - AfrE Listeners’ Identification of [a] Identification of [al Figures 15 and 16 show that AfrE listeners often confuse [a] with [ A], and [A ] with [a]- Figure 15.1 shows that 55% of Ghanaian respondents correctly identified [a], but 45% misidentified the vowel as [A] . Eighty percent of Zimbabwean respondents correctly identified [a], while 20% thought it was[A]. 55% of Kenyans had the correct 95 identification, but 35% erred. Figure 15.2 illustrate the vowel identification pattern in Figure 15.1 45 % A bud 55 % a bad (a) Ghanaians (b) Zimbabweans (C) Kenyans Figure 15.2 - AfrE Listeners’ Identification of [a] Identification of IA I It is hardly surprising that IT results shown in Figure 16 are almost identical to the ones in Figure 15. After all , these are results of an identical pair of words tested twice with the only difference being in the ordering of the pair that is, in the previous results, 96 «3195' - W, | 1 I: bud [A] Ibad [a] 1 _ (c) Kenyans (a) Ghanaians (b) Zimbabwans Figure 16.2 -- AfrE Listeners’ Identification of [A] the tested minimal pair was bad, bud , while in this case the listeners heard the word bud first. In spite of that slight change identification of [A] as low as in the first pair. 97 Only 45%, 25%, and 35% of Ghanaians, Zimbabweans, and Kenyans respectively correctly identify the vowel. 30%, 50%, and 40% of respondents, in the same order, confuse [A] with [a]. Considering the peripheral nature of L1 vowel systems of the current sample, and in particular the non-existence of central vowels, two plausible arguments can be made to account for the misperception of the two vowels. First, [A] is confiised with [a] when the native-language magnet “pulls” the non-native RP vowel [A] towards the native phoneme prototype [a]. Secondly, language contact and exposure to RP in the course of educational pursuit makes these university students more conscious of such a non-native sound as [A] . It would be interesting to test whether respondents with a lower level of education would exhibit similar vowel perceptions. Identification of I3 I A similar native language effect may be anticipated in the perception of RP’s central vowel [3], another non-existent vowel in the Lls of the present sample. However, an overview of the results presented in Figure 17 seem to invalidate such expectations in view of the fact that a majority of Ghanaians and Zimbabweans seem to 98 1:1 bird[3] El bed[e] I bad [a] 1 I I I I I I II VI II 9- VI II I I II I I III I -. r Figure 17.1 - AfrE Listeners’ Identification of [3] correctly identify the vowel. The bar chart shows that over 55% of respondents in west and southern Africa correctly identify the vowel. 20-28% of respondents in these two regions confuse the vowel with either [8 ]or [a]. However, upon closer scrutiny, it is evident that respondents who confuse [3 ]with either [8 ] or [a], are almost as many as those who correctly identify it, particularly among Zimbabweans. For instance, note that while 52% of Zimbabweans correctly identify [3 ] , the total percentage of Zimbabweans who confuse the vowel with [8] and [a] is 50%. Kenyan respondents present particularly interesting results in this case. While over 50% respondents confuse [3] with [a], no respondent confused [3] with [8]. Similar to the observation given above regarding the perception of [A], NLM may account for the variants in this case, but the identification of the non-native vowel 99 [3] may be as a result of perceptual experience through language exposure, or through 6:“ bed 20% 013m 8:332 (081mm (t) itenyans (U) Zimbabweans Figure 17.2 - AfrE Listeners’ Identification of [3] an association of the sound with similar L1 allophonic variants. [A correlation of production and perceptual results will verify these assumptions. Figure 17.2 shows the strong identification of the vowel among Ghanaians and Zimbabweans, and lowering of the vowel to /a/ by a majority of Kenyans. 11 I'll I' [[1] ’3“ .0 100 In another case of misperception, the RP vowel [I] is confused with [i] and [e], a Bid [1] I Bead [I] ll Bade [e] G1 Zim Ken Figure 18.1 - AfrE Listeners’ Identification of [I] more evidently by Kenyans and Zimbabweans than Ghanaians as Figure 18 shows. Among Kenyan listeners, 38% correctly identify [I], 42% confuse it with [e], e bade 10% (a) Ginnaians (b) Zimbabwans Figure 18.2- AfrE’s Listeners’ Identification of [I] while 35% identify it as [i]. 45% of Zimbabwean listeners correctly identify [1], 45% mistake it for [i], and 10% heard it as [e]. On the other hand, over 60% of Ghanaian 101 speakers easily identified the vowel, 30% confused it with [i], and no respondent incorrectly identified [1] as [e], as respondents from other regions did. Identification of ID | In all three regions, a majority of listeners confuse RP vowel [D] with [o], as illustrated in Figure 19.1. A low score of 36% (Ghanaians), 38% (Zimbabweans), and 38%(Kenyans) correctly identified [D], compared to 45%, 60%, and 42% (in the same order as above) who confused the vowel with [ 9]. Considering results of some earlier perception tests conducted in other English- l l 60 g‘ | j 50 g p i 40 E “fl .. r- ; i i i i — 7 l 30 o 3:3: 3;; Ilbod [n] l '3': 7 “a“ . ‘ 20 1 2:2: . , iii Iboard [o] i o 3:3: egg 1 ': iaii iii ' i i i i l . iii: ‘3' I , 3:3: ° "‘5 i G1 Zim Ken 1 | a -L Figure 19.1 - AfrE Listeners’ Identification of [D] speaking settings, it may hardly be surprising to find such a confusion among AfrE speakers. But Peterson and Barney (1953), Preston (1994) have reported such confusion only minimally among American respondents. Lieberman and Blumstein ( 1988: 179) 102 have argued that the confusion of these two vowels even in languages or dialects where the vowels are distinct in production, is due to close proximity of these vowels in the vowel space. Such a confusion among our multilingual sample may be considered a case of categorial perception of non-native sounds. The low percentage of respondents who (a) Ghanaians (b) Zimbabweans (c) Kenyans Figure 19.2 - AfrE Listeners’ Identification of [D] correctly identify RP’s [D], and the relatively higher number of the highly recognized quantal vowel [o], is discussed above. 103 Identification of | 8| B bed [e] 1: Bad [a] en Zim I Ken i l l l l l l l —— —_l Figure 20.1 - AfrE Listeners’ Identification of [8] Figure 20 shows over 60% of respondents from eastern and southern Africa correctly identify [8], while less than 30% of respondents in both regions confuse the vowel with [3]. Less than 40% of the West African sample correctly identified [8], while 38% confused it with [a]. [8] appears to be quite distinct among Kenyans and Zimbabweans. 104 (c) Kenyans (b) Zimbabweans (a) Ghanaians Figure 20.2 — AfrE Listeners’ Identification of [8] Identification of ai au and 00 Although a majority of Ghanaians confused [ai] with [a], and [au] with [a] as did ' I:lbuy[ai] l[a] Figure 21 — AfrE Listeners’ Identification of [ai] the Zimbabweans, diphthong [0g] was, however, correctly identified by most of 105 ..._ ... Zim Gt Ken I _ Figure 22 - AfrE Listeners’ Identification of [au] Ghanaian respondents as shown in Figures 21, 22 and 23. On the other hand, Kenyan respondents confused [ai] and [au] with [a], and a majority mistook [ou] for [o] as shown in Figure 21 through Figure 23. Schmied’s (1991:59) conclusion that all AfrE’s centring diphthongs tend to be pronounced as opening diphthongs or double monophthongs (/ia,ua) (see 2.2.2) is not EIboat __ [0U] ’10] Zim Gh Ken Figure 23 - AfrE’s Listeners’ Identification of [ou] supported by these perceptual results. 106 Conclusion In conclusion, vowel identification results from the MPT show that vowels [ i], [e], [o] and [u] are reliably identified by all AfrE respondents, including diphthongs containing these vowel onsets. In general, all respondents had difficulties identifying the RP vowels [A], [3 ], [D], and [I]. Regional variations are particularly evident in the perception of [3 ] , [I ] , [8]. For example, a majority of Kenyans identify [3] as [a], while among Ghanaians and Zimbabweans, [3] is correctly identified by a majority of respondents. Likewise, many Kenyans and Zimbabweans correctly identify [8], although a few confuse it with [a], but among Ghanaians, only a small percentage of respondents identified RP vowel [8]. Other variations are discussed above, but it is worth noting that these vowels are non- native to the Lls of the respondents. This is further proof that the generalized vowel systems in earlier studies are not accurate representations of the systems of the present sample. Errors evident in the perception of non-native vowels may be attributed to the native language magnetic effect postulated in the NLM theory. Social factors such as ethnicity, level of education, and age, may also account for the variation. For instance, results in the perception of RP vowels [A], [3] , [D], and [I], vowels that are generally absent in vowel systems of Kwa and Bantu language are perceived variously in the tests. 107 Suffice it to say, IT results reflect acoustic analysis discussed in chapter 3. Note for instance that RP vowel [3 ]as in bird is produced as [a] by Kenyan respondents, but Zimbabweans and Ghanaians produce it as [e]. However, we should hasten to reiterate that level of education is an important factor in the production and perception of an L2. A sample of well-educated respondents, like the present sample, may produce and perceive L2 sounds differently than the less- educated considering the inclination to RP exposure. Results of MPT task, discussed below, will provide us with a more complete picture of vowel perception. 5. 9 Results of the MPT Respondents were asked to listen to fifty-eight minimal pairs and determine whether the words were similar or not. Responses were marked on questionnaire (see Appendix D). Results were tallied and percentages calculated to determine which pairs were easily discriminated, and which ones were not (see the discrimination results in Appendix B). At this juncture, a highlight of some presuppositions of the Native Language Magnet theory will help with the analysis. Liberman (1990) observes that sounds from distinct categories are relatively easier to discriminate in MPT than non-distinct ones. The latter may be perceived as one sound. Moreover, L1 vowel categories may interfere with multilingual listeners’ ability to perceive certain phonetic distinctions of non-native L2 sounds. 108 Consequently, it is assumed in the following analysis that AfrE listeners will have difficulties discriminating vowels that are non-distinct and non-native to their L1. These adult respondents, we presume, will use their already developed L1 vowel schema in discriminating RP vowels present in the MPT. However, we hasten to add that these university students have had ample exposure to RP, but needless to say, not as much as exposure as to either AfrE or their respective Lls. This study focuses on vowels that were difficult to discriminate. Results of the MP task are illustrated in Figures 24, 25 and 26. Each figure shows the vowel pairs erroneously identified by approximately 40-50% of respondents as F2 Figure 24 - Ghanaians Discrimination of Minimal Pairs “same”, and correctly identified as “different” by a similar percentage of respondents. Shaded areas represent cases in which over 55% of respondents thought different vowel pairs were the same. 109 Figure 24 shows that Ghanaians had difficulty discriminating pairs involving vowel [A] and [3] (bud/bird); [A] and [a] (hut/hurt); [A] and [o](gut/got); [a]and [au] (bat/bout); and [u] and [U] (wood/wooed) Figure 25 shows that Kenyans, on the other hand, had difficulties discriminating vowel pairs [e] and [8], (wade/wed); [3] and [A] (bird/bud); [A] and [a] (mud/mad); [A] and [O] (gut/got); [a] and [au] (bat/bout); [O] and [o](col/caught), [au], [ua], and [on], (got/goat, bought/bout, caught/cot, law/lure). A A 4A F2 A A I i I {—5 ‘ u: e i 3 r0 2: . A IE] 0 a ‘ I Figure 25 - Kenyans Discrimination of Minimal Pairs Lastly, as shown in Figure 26, Zimbabweans had difficulties with pairs containing vowels [3] and [A] [bird/bud]; [A] and[a] (mad/mud); [o], (mud/mode); [e] and [8], (wade/wed); [a], [o],[au], [ua], and [ou], (hot/hot, gut/got, got/goat, cot/caught, call/call, bought/bout, law/lure). llO Beyond the apparent obscurity of discrimination results, there appears a clear pattern of perceptions which mirrors identification results to some degree. Consider for instance the recurrence of [a] and [o] in the lists of confused pairs in all regions, and recall the misidentification of these two vowels in the IT task. F2 fill Figure 26 - Zimbabweans Discrimination of Minimal Pairs Furthermore, RP central vowels [3] and [A] are also prominent recurrences in various pair combinations. The fact that minimal pairs containing these vowels (bird/bud, hut/hurt) are confused by many respondents in all regions, and taking cognizant of results of identification task and acoustic analysis, reaffirm that AfrE speakers and listeners realign RP central vowels in accordance with their respective L1 systems. The Westermann and Ward illustration of African Lls (see Appendix A’) affirms these observation. lll Chapter 6: Conclusions 6. 1 Introduction This study has utilized empirical tools of investigation and analysis to identify phonetic and phonemic qualities of AfrE vowel systems. Acoustic analysis has been done to determine speakers’ production of AfrE vowels. Speech perception tests have been conducted to determine listeners’ abstract realization of those vowels. Several theoretical assumptions guided us in the present analysis of the data. This concluding chapter seeks to tease out a probably phonemic interpretation of these vowel systems by correlating vowel production and perception results presented in the previous chapters. Although it is difficult to state categorically that the results represent the actual phonemes of the vowel systems of a given multilingual region, acoustic and perceptual analysis indicate, among other things, a variance in vowel production and perception across regions and ethnic groups studied. The results contradict some of the regional variations widely documented in previous studies. For example, Schmied (1991) and other scholars, have consistently argued that the major source of AfrE variation is due to regional variance in the production of the RP central vowel [3] (as in bird). As stated earlier, these scholars claims that [:3] backs to [0] in WAfrE, fronts and lowers to [a] or fronts to [e] in EAfrE, while in SAfrE it fronts to [e]. As discussed in the following sections, data from this sample does not corroborate this theoretical premise, especially in regard to what the acoustic and perceptual realizations of the central vowels are in each region. Furthermore the identical vowel production among eastern and southern Africa speakers, that is claimed to constitute Bantu English, is also not corroborated . 112 This study also found a distinction between male and female vowel system, particularly in the acoustic analysis. Such a distinction merits further investigation with a view to understanding vowel variance within and across social groups in each region. For instance, what are the social factors that account for these gender distinctions? Is the distinction indicative of any changes in progress in the vowel production of the varieties we examined? And what is direction of the change, if any? On the theoretical front, the current study has corroborated assumptions that there indeed is an underlying AfrE vowel system, as well as distinct regional variations. Furthermore, comparing Westermann and Ward’s illustration of L1 vowel system and the results derived in this study, it is apparent that the tenets of the Native Language Magnet theory apply in our respondents’ perception of non-native vowels. Let us attempt to correlate production and perception results, and determine how we reached these conclusions. 6. 2 Correlating Acoustic and Perceptual Results Vowel production and perceptual data has unequivocally shown that vowels / il, /o/, and /u/ and diphthongs that contain similar onsets are contrastive, easily identified and discriminated without difficulties across the board. AfrE speakers and listeners realized these phones as allophonic variants; [i]/[I], [u]/[U], [Al/[a] and [D]/[o]. Applying Lieberman’s physiological theory for vowels, we can assume that these vowels have well defined spectral properties, and are therefore, AfrE’s quantal vowels which define the vowel space within which AfrE speakers and listeners (represented in this study) differentiate other English vowels. Moreover, based on principles of Native 113 Language Magnet theory, we can further argue that these and other identified regional phonemes (discussed below), are in effect influenced by the L1 of these multilingual speakers. However, we should hasten to add regional differences are evident in the production and/or perception of [3 ], [I], [e], [8] and other diphthongs . In the following sections, we will seek to correlate production and perception results by restating the main results and subsequently discussing them. Ghanaians Although there were gender variations in vowel quality, vowel production among Ghanaians was such that vowels in tokens bead , hid, hue, and here were realized as [i]; year and hair as [e]; and bed, name, bird as [8]; hard, pride, bout, bad and mud as [a]; sad, caught and board are pronounced as [a]; open, goat, sure, and boy were realized as [0]; while mood and good were realized as [u]. The only apparent gender distinction in vowel production among our sample lies in the realization of [ua] as in sure. While male respondents realize the monophthongized diphthong as [0], females pronounce it as [u]. Speech perception tests reflect production results, considering in particular the high identification rate of vowels [i], [o], and [u], and diphthongs that contain similar onsets. For example, in the acoustic analysis, F1 and F2 scores of diphthongs contained in hue, year load in the phonetic space of /i/. As stated above allophonic variants [a]/[A] and [a]/[o] pairs are not clearly identified, and are not easily discriminated, because, as the production results indicate, they are not distinct vowels. [A]is produced as [a] while [a] is produced as [o]. The first 114 mentioned change is particularly interesting considering that earlier studies claimed that WAfrE speakers realized RP’s [A] is as [a]. RP central vowel [3], is produced as [8], identified as such by 23% of respondents, perceive as [a] by 20%, and difficulties were evident in discriminating [3]/[A] pair. We have already established that [A] is indeed realized as [a], so in the last mentioned pair it should be read as such. Again, what is particularly interesting in this finding is that, production and perceptual evidence does not corroborate widely documented claim that WAfrE respondents back the central vowel to [0], at least not with this sample. Based on production and perceptual evidence, RP / 3/ may be considered an allophone of /8/ among our Ghanaian respondents. Monophthongized lai/ and /au/ are allophones of /a/. Zimbabweans Zimbabweans produce vowels contained in the tokens bead, hid as [i]; year and hair as [e]; made, bet, name, bad , bird as [8]; hard, pride, bout, and mud as [a]; sod, open, goat, caught, board, sure, and boy as [0]; while mood and good are realized as [u]. Male respondents have a lax and tense distinction for the front mid vowel while female speakers do not. Moreover, while male respondents front RP’s central vowel [3] as in bird to [8], and raise the low back vowel [a] to [a], female respondents barely front [3] as in bird to [8], but they do not raise [a]. 115 Results of the identification task indicate that an equal number of Zimbabwean listeners confused [c] with [ei]. Moreover, 66% of the respondents perceived made/made pair as different words in the discrimination task. Gender distinction is also evident in male respondents’ lax/tense distinction of the front mid vowel. Moreover, while male respondents front RP’s central vowel [3] as in bird to [e], and raise low back vowel [a] to [a], female respondents only front [3] as in bird to [8], but they do not raise [a]. The distinction may account for the identification results which shows that 30% of respondents perceive [3] as [ a] and 20% as [e]. The variation is also reflected in the difficulties in discriminating bat/bat and bird/bud pairs. Previous studies have categorically stated that SAfrE speakers front [3] to [e], and [e] is realized as [e]. It is plausible that le/ has many variants in the English variety spoken by our Zimbabwean sample, including /e/, /8/, and /ea/. Monophthongization of [ai] and [au] to [a], and [ou] to [o], is evident in production and discrimination results. Kenxans The Kenyan sample produced vowels contained in the tokens bead and hid as [i]; vowels in the words made and bet as [e]; year, hair, and name , as [8]; bad, hard, pride, bout, bird and mud as [a]; sad, open, goat, caught, board and boy as [0]; while mood , good and sure are realized as [u]. It should be noted that while males make [e] — [8] distinction, females do not. The only other significant variation is that while male respondents realize [19] as /e/, female speakers produce it as /i/. 116 The production of [3] as [a] is reflected in identification task where 52% of respondents mistake [3] for [a], a variation quite distinct from Ghanaians’ and Zimbaweans’. Moreover, unlike in the other two varieties, no Kenyan respondent identified [3] as [8]. /3/ in Kenyan English is an allophone of /a/, and not /e/ as the case among Zimbabweans and Ghanaians. It is also interesting to note that, unlike identification responses from the other regions, over 63% of Kenyan respondents perceive RP [8] as [a]. The distinctions between varieties spoken by Zimbabweans and Kenyans are worth noting considering earlier claims that the varieties are almost identical except for variation in the realization of [3]. Is it possible then to argue that /8/ is an allophone of la]? Monophthongization of [ai] and [au] to [a], and [ou] to [o], is reflected in production and discrimination results. We should hasten to add, these results do not represent the vowel systems of speakers of national or regional varieties of AfrE. Our sample was drawn from well- educated, Kwa and Bantu language speakers, and can only be considered to represent that particular sample. Whether the results represent the broader population of Kwa and Bantu speakers, or national varieties, will be determined after similar studies have been conducted covering other social, ethnic and language omitted in this study. Future research should utilize empirical tools of research adapted in this study to determine AfrE vowel production and perception among respondents of different ethnic, social and linguistic backgrounds. ll7 6. 3 Theoretical Assumptions Revisited Based on our empirical findings, several observations can be made regarding theoretical assumptions about AfrE, speech production and perception, and about the Native Language Magnet (NLM) theory. (a) Assumptions about AfrE In this study African English was regarded as a very general term that refers to divergent social, regional and ethnic sub-varieties of English in East, West and Southern Africa. The regional varieties of AfrE (EAfrE, WAfrE, SAfrE) were also perceived in very general terms as diverse sub-varieties that have distinct regional characteristics, as Mutonya’s ( 1997) attitudinal study of AfrE varieties showed. Results of acoustic measurements and perception tests discussed in this study, show that although each regional vowel system has its own distinctive (marked) elements, there are some basic similarities (unmarked) in all regional vowel systems that presuppose an underlying AfrE vowel system. The marked elements account for the individual Kenyan-Bantu, Zimbabwean- Bantu, and Ghanaian-Kwa (Akan) vowel systems, while the unmarked ones represent the underlying AfrE vowel system. For instance, the acoustic analyses shows that Ghanaian and Zimbabwean speakers of English front the RP vowel [3 ] to [e] , while Kenyan speakers lower it to [a]. Furthermore, the central vowel [A ] as in bud is lowered by Kenyans and Ghanaian speakers, but Zimbabwean speakers realize it as [e]. These are regional variations. 118 However, in spite of such regional variations , it is evident that all speakers have a peripheral vowel system that consists of /i/, /e/ (and /8/ in some instances), /a/, /o/ and /u/, similar to Westermann and Ward’s illustration of African L1 vowels. RP’s central vowels /3/ and /A / are lowered or fronted, while RP vowel contrast is lost in /i/-/I/, /a/- /A/, /a/-/a/, /:>/ - /o/, and /u/-/U/. These are general characteristics that transcend all three samples in this study. Similarly, vowel perception results in this study reflect both general and region specific traits. For example, in the IT task all listeners confused words containing the vowels /3, A, o, 8 , c1 /. The misidentification pattern is almost identical in all regions, except for the specifics of whether a particular vowel was strongly or weakly. Consider for instance that while RP /3/ is also misidentified as /e/ and /a/ in all regions, 58% of Ghanaians and 55% of Zimbabweans positively identify the vowel, while only 32% of Kenyan identified it correctly. 55% of Kenyans misidentified the vowel as /a/ while only 30% and 18% of Zimbabweans and Ghanaians respectively, identified it as such. In order to avoid methodological shortcomings in analyzing AfrE varieties, use of controlled samples and empirical tools of research is the best way of determining the regional characteristics of AfrE . (b) Assumptions Concerning Speech Production and Perception Lieberman (1990) reports that experiments point to the fact that listeners perceive speech categorically; that is , listeners’ ability to discriminate sounds from two different 119 categories, such as /i/ and /e/, should be relatively easy, but discriminating two different tokens of /i/ from the same speaker should be difficult. The findings in this study support the assumption that vowels are perceived categorically, considering that results of the MPT task reflect difficulties in discrimination of non-distinct sounds. For example, Kenyan listeners had difficulties discriminating sounds that are non-distinct in Bantu languages [A] / [ 3 ], [A] / [a], [A] / [Ct ] , and [a] / [ au] . Acoustic analysis suggests the Kenyan sample realize all these phones as variants of /a/. Furthermore [e]/ [ 8] and [u]/[U] are also difficult to discriminate because, as the acoustic analysis points out, they are variants, and presumably allophones of /e/ and /u/ respectively. Similar examples from other respondents can be cited. The non-distinct vowels that are difficult to discriminate in the perception tests are also non-distinct vowels in acoustic analysis. Correlating speech production and perception is based on the consideration that every speaker is simultaneously a listener, and every listener is at least potentially a speaken Suffice it to say, the scope and design of this study does not provide us the evidence to verify the main assumptions of the Motor Theory of Speech Perception: that listeners’ interpretation of acoustic signals are guided by articulatory gestures . Similarly, we cannot empirically determine whether listeners use vocal tract normalization during vowel perception, although categorical perception of speech, discussed above, points towards that direction. 120 (c) Assumptions about N on-native Speech Perception Recall that the NLM theory postulates that adult listeners perceive non-native sound contrasts based on a schema developed from their native language. The native phonological system of the listener presumably plays a prominent role in perception of non-native sound contrasts. Several factors make it difficult for us to ascertain whether listeners’ L1 vowel systems affect their perception of RP vowels. The lack of empirically defined vowel systems of Kwa and Bantu languages denies us reliable comparative evidence. However, based on widely documented information, including Westermann and Wards’ study ( discussed in Chapter 3), Bantu and Akan vowel systems are peripheral and symmetrical. The L1 systems have five to seven vowels and no central vowels (Welmerszl988). Such characteristics are reflected in the perception of RP vowels in this study. Furthermore, results of the perceptual tests indicating RP’s contrastive vowels are perceived as non- contrastive may be indicative of the L1 effect. The reduction in number of the RP vowels in production and perception by these AfrE respondents may point towards the same conclusion. However, although it is logical to assume an L1 influence in production and perception of non-native sounds, considering that adult speech perception is organized to process L1 with the least effort and greatest efficiency, it is equally appropriate to argue from a sociolinguistics standpoint that social and other linguistic factors may play significant roles in speech perception besides L1 effect. For example, in multilingual sub-Saharan Africa, level of education is closely related to degree of exposure and usage of English, just as much as social status is. 121 Evaluating speech perception of multilingual respondents in sub-saharan Africa, leaves us wondering how factors such as language contact, listener’s exposure and attitude to RP, level of education, multi-lingualism, influence respondents’ perception besides the L1 effect. Since most of our respondents are fluent in other local languages, It would be interesting to determine whether the indigenous L25 and L3s of these multilingual listeners affect their perceptions as much as L 1 does. Would a similar sample, with a relatively lower level of education have similar vowel perception? How about a respondent who has a lower level of education but a higher degree of exposure and interaction with RP speakers? Would a respondent who has a strong negative attitude towards English perceive RP vowels any different from his counterpart who is positively inclined towards English? Only further research can help us answer these and other lingering questions . Finally, this study has shown that the maximally distinct AfrE vowels are [i], [e], [o] and [u]. Studies among English monolinguals had identified [i], [11] ad [a] as the quantal vowels. Future studies should build on the foundations of this research in the seeking to determine whether empirical evidence supports the broad definitions of varieties of African Englishes. Furthermore, the tools of investigation and analysis used in this study can be utilized in the empirical research of African languages. 122 APPENDICES 123 APPENDIX A’ 124 APPENDIX A’ (i): Cardinal Vowel Chart Front central back (1 i (8) U (2) (7)0 e (3) 8 r (6) o (4) a. ‘* (5) 0 APPENDIX A’(ii): Westerman’s Illustration of RP vowels Front 0 entral back i u e I \Ejfwuo a D 125 APPENDIX A TABLE 1: A PROFILE OF RESPONDENTS Ll wendwa utia ukami an' ukusu eru aita an'iru ones hona Nenziwe hona .Nenziwe hona debele afumo ' akawira hona debele ovele hona hona ukulu hona debele debele 126 127 APPENDIX B 128 APPENDIX Bl AN EXAMPLE OF A N ORMALIZED DATA FILE Maina,20,M,Formal,Urban, Kenya 22,6.677709 314,2289,,1.l42,1,hid 283,2207,,1 1.1421,1,bead 453,2333,,l4.00091,1,year 415,1812,,2.1411,1,bet 456,1750,,21.1422,1,made 493,2094,,21.4124,1,name 565,1731,,24.6,1,hair 663,1480,,3.1421,1,bad 683,1273,,42.1411,1,bout 678,1563,.44.6,1,hard 655,1716,,47.1428,1,pride 534,1114,,5.1423,1,sod 469,996,,53.1416,1,caught 670,1508,,6.1422,1,mud 547,1008,,61.0001,1,boy 472,915,,62. 1416,1,goat 503,996,,63.1 1 101,1,open 470,913 ,,64.6001,l,board 297,865,,7.1426,1,good 3 17,823,,72.1422,1,mood 426,915,,74.6003,1,sure 670,1334,,94.6001,1,bird 129 Ghanaians All Ghanaians (20 M&F) 473 372,2073,,1.l42,1,hid 380,2003,,ll.l421,l,bead 387.1988,,14.00091,l,year 640,1952,,2.14ll,l,bet 566,1877,,21.1422,1,made 439,2152,,21.4124,1,name 506,1930,,24.6,1,hair 841,1564,,3.l421,l,bad 640,1449,,42.14ll,l,bout 789,119l,,44.6,1,hard 655,1580,,47.l428,1,pride 500,1213,,5.l423,l,sod 633,946,,53.l416,1,caught 767,1266,,6.l422,l,mud 484,968,,61.0001,l,boy 365,1058,,62.1416,1,goat 439,1024,,63.11101,l,open 528,789,,64.6001,1,board 314,1058,,7.l426,l,good 305,805,,72.1422,l,mood 455,1275,,74.6003,1,sure 603,l787,,94.6001,l,bird 478,2894,,l.142,l,hid 494,2694,,ll.l421,1,bead 424,2825,,l4.6,l,here 670,2331,,2.1421,1,bed 463,2725,,21.1422,l,made 617,2523,,24.6,1,hair 810,1173,,3.1421,1,bad 787,1335,,4l.0001,1,buy 525,941,,42.14ll,1,bout 988,1282,,44.6,l,hard 701,1223,,5.l424,l,nod 887,1319,,6.1422,1,mud 648,1103,,61.0001,1,boy 470,1065,,62.14ll,1,boat 602,104l,,63.0003,1,saw 455,1046,,7.1426,1,good 378,1073,,72.1422,l,mood 563,1266,,74.6003,l,sure 578,1119,,74.6003,l,tour 463,2040,,82.,l,hue 663,1976,,94.6001,l,bird 309,2724,,1.142,l,hid 426,2607,,1l.1421,1,bead 382,2430,,14.6,1,here 603,2407,,2.1421,1,bed 463,2547,,21.1422,l,made 662.2407,,24.6,l,hair 832,1077,,3.l42l,1,bad 721,1760,,4l.0001,l,buy APPENDIX B2 ALL Fl/FZ SCORES 758,1641,,42.1411,1,bout 964,1561,,44.6,1,hard 730,967,,5.1424,1,nod 880,1308,,6.1422,l,mud 603,949,,61.0001,1,boy 522,1082,,62.1411,1,b0at 603,1178,,63.0003,1,saw 434,1031,,7.1426,1,good 476,995,,72.l422,1,mood 453.1207,,74.6003,1,sure 625,1126,,74.6003,1,tour 471,2229,,82.,1,hue 546,2371,,94.6001,l,bird 413.2797,,l.l42,l,hid 429,2859,,11.1421,1,bead 444,2629,,l4.6,l,here 581.2452,,2.1421,1,bed 406,2598,,21.1422,1,made 574,2537,,24.6,1,hair 950,1188,,3.1421,1,bad 864,1141,,41.0001,1,buy 886,1177,,42.1411,1,bout 1019,1547,,44.6,1,hard 602,1072,,5.1424,1,nod 851,1238,,6.1422,1,mud 620,1019,,61.0001,1yboy 528,989,,62.l4ll,1,boat 574,1088,,63.0003,1,saw 421,llll,,7.1426,l,good 368,1088,,72.l422,1,mood 505.1402,,74.6003,1,sure 543,1004,,74.6003,l,tour 375,2721,,82.,1,hue 628,2414,,94.6001,l,bird 515,2529,,1.l42,1,hid 393,2668,,1l.l421,1,bead 422,2885,,14.6,1,here S97,2055,,2.l421,l,bed 437,2572,,21.1422,1,made 611,2368,,24.6,l,hair 802,1683,,3.1421,1,bad 918,1522,,41.0001,1,buy 757,1174,,42.14ll,l,bout 779,1261,,44.6,1,hard 743,1166,,5.1424,1,nod 765,1347,,6.1422,1,mud 130 611,1014,,6l.0001,l,boy 437,815,,62.14ll,l,boat 648.1137,,63.0003,1,saw 415,1003,,7.1426,1,good 453,966,,72.l422,l,mood 435,1294,,74.6003,l,sure 685,1075,,74.6003,1,tour 459,2471,,82.,1,hue 662,2318,,94.6001,1,bird 468,2352,,1.l42,1,hid 389,2583,,11.1421,1,bead 447,2294,,14.6,1,here 592,2265,,2.l421,l,bed 432,2425,,21.1422,1,made 649,2234,,24.6,l,hair 715,1060,,3.l421,1,bad 772,1082,,4l.0001,1,buy 830,1414,,42.14ll,1,bout 959,1443,,44.6,1,hard 728,1046,,5.1424,l,nod 894,1522,,6.1422,1,mud 605,880,,61.0001,1,boy 541,1002,,62.l411,1,boat 618,1024,,63.0003,1,saw 455,902,,7.l426,1,good 411,959,,72.l422,1,mood 560,1215,,74.6003,1,sure 605,1103,,74.6003,1,tour 426,2388,,82.,l,hue 605.2814,,94.6001,1,bird 435,2716,,1.142,1,hid 419,2760,,11.1421,1,bead 4SO,2851.,14.6,1,here 472,2316,,2.1421,1,bed 442,2950,,21.l422,1,made 711,2507,.24.6,1,hair 789,1180,,3.1421,1,bad 840,1180,,41.0001,1,buy 738,1335,,42.14ll,1,bout 862,1220,,44.6,l,hard 759,1141,,5.1424,1,nod 83l,1318,,6.1422,l,mud 701,106l,,6l.0001,1,boy 487,893,,62.l4ll,1,boat 612,1055,,63.0003,1,saw 408,974,,7.1426,l,good 465,975,,72.1422,l,mood 465.1299,,74.6003,1,sure 671,1055,,74.6003,l,tour 450,2583,,82.,1,hue 585,2359,,94.6001,l,bird 440,276l,.l.l42,1,hid 360,2841,,11.l421,1,bead 440.2885,,14.6,1,here 639,2452,,2.1421,1,bed 418,2836,,21.1422,l,made 647,2547,,24.6,1,hair 821,1123,,3.1421,1,bad 793,1123,,4l.0001,1,buy 785,1212,,42.l411,1,bout 895,1186,,44.6,l,hard 778.1167,,5.1424,1,nod 815,1182,,6.l422,1,mud 675.1087,,61.0001,1,boy 417,829,,62.14ll,1,boat 698,1123,,63.0003,l,saw 410,1020,,7.1426,1,good 46S,969,,72.1422,1,mood 498,1301,,74.6003,1,sure 690,1064,,74.6003,1,tour 359,2724,,82.,1,hue 682,2496,,94.6001,l,bird 493.2507,,1.142,l,hid 440,2772,,11.1421,1,bead 479,2203,,14.6,l,here 585.2187,,2.1421,1,bed 511,2630,,21.1422,1,made 673,2126,,24.6,1,hair 805,1192,,3.1421,1,bad 790,1132,,4l.0001,1,buy 766.1557,,42.l411,1,bout 896,1298,,44.6,l,hard 736,1101,,5.1424,1,nod 1010,1336,,6.l422,1,mud 587,896,,61.0001,1,boy 531.1086,,62.l4ll,l,boat 576,1033,,63.0003,1,saw 440,934,,7.l426,1,good 450,995,,72.1422,1,mood S31,1314,,74.6003,1,sure S42,1086,,74.6003,1,tour 448,2423,,82.,l,hue 622,2218,,94.6001,1,bird 453,2500,,1.142,1,hid 422,2832,,11.l421,1,bead 392,2569,,14.6,1,here 566,2288,,2.l421,1,bed 446,2508,,21.l422,1,made 616,2167,,24.6,1,hair 860,1812,,3.l421,l,bad 84S,1631,,41.0001,1,buy 845,1623,,42.l411,1,bout 875,1683,,44.6,1,hard 641,1004,,5.1424,1,nod 896,1563,,6.1422,1,mud 634,955,,61.0001,1,boy 430,951,,62.l4ll,l,boat 649,1004,,63.0003,1,saw 378,831,,7.1426,l,good 468,997,,72.l422,1,mood 453,1254,,74.6003,1,sure 649,1012,,74.6003,1,tour 354,2483,,82.,1,hue 581,2251,,94.6001,1,bird 424,2588,,1.l42,1,hid 427,2743,,11.1421,1,bead 440,2498,,l4.6,l,here 537,2386,,2.l421,1,bed 533,2559,,21.l422,l,made 66l,2153,,24.6,l,hair 912,1794,,3.l421,l,bad 87S,l690,,41.0001,1,buy 823,1457,,42.1411,l,b0ut 882,1129,,44.6,1,hard 508,935,,5.1424,1,n0d 905,124l,,6.1422,1,mud 569.1158,,6l.0001,1,b0y 503,965,,62.1411,l,boat 621,2421,,24.6,1,hair 953,1900,,3.1421,1,bad 873,1430,,41.0001,1,buy 408,983,,42.14ll.l,bout 907,1188,,44.6,1,hard 741,1173,,5.l424,l,nod 787,1127,,6.l422,l,mud 665,1029,,61.0001,1,boy 438,840,,62.l4ll,1,boat 711,1152,,63.0003,l,saw 446,1036,,7.l426,1,good 4S3,975,,72.l422,1,mood 453,1270,,74.6003,l,sure 680,1180,,74.6003,1,tour 370,2784,,82.,1,hue 589,2406,,94.6001,1,bird 354,226l,,1.142,1,hid 325,2255,,ll.l421,1,bead 517.2329,,14.0009l,l,year 517,1804.,2.l411,1,bet 458,2114,,21.l422,l,made 445.2047,,2l.4124,l,name 509,1942,,24.6,1,hair 739,1485,,3.1421,1,bad 694,116l,,42.14ll,1,bout 658,1471,,44.6,1,hard 576.1457,,47.l428,l,pride 531,1042,,5.1423,1,sod 557,1094,,53.l416,l,caught 820,1434,,6.1422,1,mud 463,968,,61.0001,1,boy 481,1064,,62.14l6,l,goat 435,858,,63.11101,1,open 495.1124,,64.600l,l,board 458,880,,7.1426,1,good 413,805,,72.l422,l,mood 443,1086,,74.6003,1,sure Sl7,l779,,94.6001,l,bird 326,2193,,l.l42,l,hid 288,2382,,11.l421,l,bead 531.2028,,14.00091,1,year 531,1755,,2.14ll,l,bet 303,2481,,21.l422,1,made 4S7,2192,,21.4124,1,name 523,1966,,24.6,1,hair 647,109S,,63.0003,1,saw 395,1054,,7.l426,1,good 463,1002,,72.1422,1,mood 440,1353,,74.6003,1,sure 530,1046,,74.6003,l,tour 372,2398,,82.,1,hue 624,2393,,94.6001,1,bird 423,2883,,1.l42,l,hid 370,2989,,11.l421,1,bead 431,293S,,14.6,1,here 560,2520,,2.1421,1,bed 438,2921,,21.1422,1,made 614,1834,,3.l421,l,bad 713,1273,,42.14ll.1,bout 773,1404,,44.6,1,hard 713.1381,,47.1428,1,pride 508,956,,5.1423,1,sod 577,948,,53.1416,1,caught 827,1366,,6.1422,1,mud 493,986,,61.0001,1,boy 493,1078,,62.14l6,1,goat 429,836,,63.11101,1,open 552.1292,,64.6001,1,b0ard 448,971,,7.1426,1,good 351,873,,72.l422,1,mood 475,1079,,74.6003,l,sure 531,1548,,94.6001,1,bird 37l,2108,,1.142,1,hid 386,2282,,11.1421,l,bead 393,2375,,14.0009l,1,year 454,1835,,2.1411,1,bet 364,2214,,21.1422,1,made 447,2101,,21.4124,1,name 515.1847,,24.6,1,hair 720,1326,,3.1421,l,bad 659,1510,,42.1411,1,bout 811,1394,,44.6,l,hard 690.1342,,47.1428,l,pride 478,1213,,5.1423,l,sod 546.1206,,53.l416,1,caught 819,1228,,6.1422,1,mud 500,1008,,61.0001,l,boy 436.1067,,62.1416,1,goat 132 409,909,,63.11101,l,open 470,894,,64.6001,1,board 350,1106,,7.1426,1,good 345,925,,72.1422,1,mood 453,1308,,74.6003,l,sure 530.1888,,94.6001.1,bird 429,223l,,l.142,1,hid 370,2105,,11.1421,l,bead 416.2194,,14.00091,1,year 497,183l,,2.1411,1,bet 422,2105,,21.1422,1,made 441,2148,,21.4124,1,name 506,2001,,24.6,1,hair 734,1393,,3.1421,1,bad 667.1527,,42.14ll,1,bout 768,1400,,44.6,l,hard 655,1528,,47.1428,l,pride 555,1118,,5.1423,1,sod 607.1141,,53.1416,1,caught 711,1149,,6.l422,1,mud 465,1046,,61.0001,l,boy 416,989,,62.l416,1,goat 437,1019,,63.11101,l,open 540.1037,,64.6001,l,board 303,904,,7.l426,1,good 318,948,,72.l422,1,mood 451,1196,,74.6003,l,sure 440,2315,,21.1422,1,made 426,2116,,21.4124,1,name 503,2043,,24.6,1,hair 705.1266,,3.1421,1,bad 645,1484,,42.1411,1,bout 712,1294,,44.6,l,hard 687,1328,,47.1428,1,pride 494,1197,,5.1423,1,sod 588,941,,53.14l6,l,caught 66l,l455,,6.1422,1,mud 487,970,,61.0001,1,boy 481,1034,,62.1416,l,goat 536,955,,63.11101,1,open 511,992,,64.6001,1,board 413,950,,7.1426,1,good 370,1018,,72.1422,1,mood 451,1197,,74.6003,l,sure 603,l9ll,,94.6001,1,bird 36l,2064,,1.l42,1,hid 349,2220,,11.1421,1,bead 402.2086,,l4.0009l,1,year 520,187l,,2.1411,1,bet 550.1880,,21.1422,l,made 452,2095,,21.4124,1,name 497,1983,,24.6,1,hair 653,1470,,3.l421,l,bad 683,1448,,42.l411,1,bout 733,1433,,44.6,l,hard 684.1317,,47.l428,l,pride 578,1188,,5.l423,1,sod 532.1077,,53.1416,l,caught 504,1979,,94.6001,l,bird 391,2346,,1.142,1,hid 39l,2339,,ll.1421,1,bead 434.2224,,14.0009l,l,year 523.1793,,2.1411.1,bet 427,2184,,21.1422,1,made 446,2089,,21.4124,1,name 4S7,2044,,24.6,1,hair 774,1210,,3.1421,1,bad 690,1476,,42.1411,1,bout 848,1291,,44.6,1,hard 578.1458,,47.1428,1,pride 568,1070,,5.1423,1,sod 576,974,,53.14l6,1,caught 716,1188,,6.1422,l,mud 484,979,,61.0001,1,boy 460,1012,,62.1416,l,goat 442,929,,63.11101,1,open 508,855,,64.6001,1,board 413,958,,7.1426,1,good 413,807,,72.1422,1,mood 444,1263,,74.6003,1,sure 532.1904,,94.6001,1,bird 375.2227,,1.142,1,hid 395,2279,,11.1421,1,bead 381.1958,,14.00091,1,year 508,1943,,2.1411,l,bet 720,1419,,6.1422,1,mud 465,960,,61.0001,l,boy 435,1006,,62.1416,1,goat 430,928,,63.11101,1,open 556,995,,64.6001,l,board 327,854,,7.l426,l,good 354,1032,,72.l422,1,mood 437,1175,,74.6003,l,sure 542,1813,,94.6001,l,bird 346,2117,,l.l42,l,hid 323,2048,,ll.l421,l,bead 401,2052,,l4.0009l,l,year 570,1770,,2.l4ll,l,bet 392,2062,,21.1422,1,made 453,2215,,21.4124,1,name 537,2054,,24.6,l,hair 754,1424,,3.1421,l,bad 723,1532,,42.14ll,l,bout 723,1331,,44.6,1,hard 599.1362,,47.1428,l,pride 56l,1047,,5.l423,l,sod 588,862,,53.l4l6,l,caught 685,1200,,6.l422,1,mud 491,1097,,6l.0001,l,boy 490,1089,,62.l4l6.l,goat 484.1025,,63.lllOl,1,open 523,854,,64.6001,1,board 396,1009,,7.l426,l,good 339,961,,72.l422,l,mood 483,1312,,74.6003,1,sure 592,1801,,94.6001,1,bird 325,219l,,l.l42,l.hid 368,2182,,11.1421,l,bead 518.2269,,14.00091,1,year 515,1796,,2.l4ll,l,bet 386,2069,,21.l422,l,made 444,2125,,21.4124,l,name 512.1982,,24.6,l,hair 675,1342,,3.l421,1,bad 680,1558,,42.14ll,1,bout Kenyans All Kenyans (20 M&F) 242 27l,2616,,1.142.l,hid 244,2438,,11.1421,1,bead 542.1973,,l4.0009l,l,year 531.1877,,2.1411,l,bet 452.1728,,21.l422,1,made 548,1956,,21.4124,1,name 512,1660,,24.6,l,hair 694.1607,,3.1421,1,bad 683,1249,,42.14ll,l,bout 714,1177,,44.6,l,hard 633,1445,,47.1428,1,pride 557.1007,,5.1423,1,sod 466.1025,,53.l416,1,caught 762,1500,,6.1422,1,mud 475,918,,61.0001,l,boy 535,971,,62.1416,1,goat 465,971,,63.11101,l,open 427,789,,64.6001,1,board 682,1281,,44.6,1,hard 690.1494,,47.1428,1,pride 553.1145,,5.l423,l,sod 591.1206,,53.l4l6,1,caught 743,1326,,6.l422,1,mud 454,987,,61.0001,l.boy 494,1025,,62.1416,1,goat 439.1008,,63.11101,1,open 462,834,,64.6001,1,board 36l,1008,,7.1426,l,good 37l,939,,72.1422,1,mood 453,1292,,74.6003,l,sure 515,1706,,94.6001,1,bird 300,2203,,1.142,l,hid 354,2203,,11.1421,1,bead 508.2201,,14.00091,1,year 544.1897,,2.1411,1,bet 393,1968,,21.1422,1,made 44l,2030,,21.4124,1,name 502,1918,,24.6,1,hair 733,1459,,3.1421,1,bad 684,1458,,42.1411,1,bout 720,1459,,44.6,l,hard 641.1328,,47.1428,1,pride 541,1022,,5.1423,1,sod 583.1123,,53.1416,1,caught 662,1101,,6.1422,l,mud 486,955,,61.0001,1,boy 465,1041,,62.l416,1,goat Sll,948,,63.11101,l,open 483.1160,,64.6001,l,board 381,1025,,7.1426,1,good 388,948,,72.1422,1,mood 4Sl,1241,,74.6003,1,sure 496.1897,,94.6001,1,bird 353,878,,7.l426,1,good 326,810,,72.l422,1,mood 434,1238,,74.6003,1,sure S98,1518,,94.6001,1,bird 314,2289,,1.142,1,hid 283,2207,,11.1421,1,bead 453.2333,,14.00091,1,year 415,1812,,2.1411,1,bet 456,1750,,21.1422,1,made 493,2094,,21.4124,l,name S65,l731,,24.6,1,hair 663,1480,,3.1421,1,bad 683,1273,,42.l4ll,l,bout 678,1563,,44.6,1,hard 655.1716,,47.1428,1,pride 534,1114,,5.1423,1,sod 469,996,,53.l416,1,caught 670,1508,,6.1422,1,mud 547,1008,,61.0001,1,boy 472,915,,62.14l6,1,goat 503,996,,63.11101,1,open 134 470,913,,64.6001,1,board 297,865,,7.1426,1,good 317,823,,72.1422,1,mood 426,915,,74.6003,l,sure 670,1334,,94.6001,1,bird 311,193l,,1.142,l,hid 311,1877,,ll.l421,l,bead 563.1924,,14.00091,1,year 536,187l,,2.14ll,1,bet 469,1924,,21.1422,l,made 535,1982,,21.4124,1,name 411,1657,,24.6,1,hair 717,1267,,3.142l,1,bad 674,1226,,42.l411,l,bout 707,1468,,44.6,1,hard 656.1653,,47.1428,1,pride 577,1140,,5.1423,1,sod 529,992,,53.14l6,l,caught 764,1253,,6.1422,1,mud 474,930,,61.0001,l,boy 483,966,,62.l416,1,goat 488,961,,63.11101,l,open 494,858,,64.6001,1,board 37S,972,,7.l426,1,good 390,918,,72.1422,1,mood 394,887,,74.6003,1,sure 691,1281,,94.6001,1,bird 479,2101,,1.142,l,hid 315,2239,,ll.l421,l,bead 373.2085,,l4.0009l,1,year 485,179l,,2.l411,1,bet 4S9,2219,,21.l422,1,made 484,1949,,21.4124,1,name 524.1616,,24.6,l,hair 735.1582,,3.1421,l,bad 640,1209,,42.14ll,1,bout 748,1445,,44.6,1,hard 671,1650,,47.l428,1,pride 551.1254,,5.1423,1,sod 556,932,,53.l4l6,l,caught 643,1299,,6.l422,1,mud 474,955,,61.0001,l,boy 473,887,,62.l4l6,l,goat 502,890,,63.11101,1,open 554,715,,64.6001,1,board 310,682,,7.1426,1,good 347,610,,72.1422,1,mood 432,975.,74.6003,1,sure 794,1555,,94.6001,l,bird 347,2095,,1.l42,1,hid 308,2148,,1l.1421,1,bead 353,2158,,14.00091,1,year 542,1833,,2.l4ll,l,bet 636,1854,,21.1422,1,made 474,1939,,21.4124,1,name 522.1648,,24.6,l,hair 636,1150,,3.1421,1,bad 647,1223,,42.14ll,1,bout 685,1198,,44.6,1,hard 688.1567,,47.1428,1,pride 522.1077,,5.l423,1,sod 568,1030,,53.l4l6,1,caught 702,1191,,6.1422,1,mud 543,1157,,61.0001,1,boy 471,930,,62.1416,l,goat 485,971,,63.11101,1,open 535,923,,64.6001,1,board 381,843,,7.1426,1,good 407,756,,72.1422,1,mood 450,1020,,74.6003,1,sure 648,1097,,94.6001,1,bird 369,1950,,l.142,1,hid 313,2096,,1l.1421,1,bead 492,2043,,14.00091,1,year 480,1615,,2.l411,l,bet 431,1755,,21.1422,l,made 515,2036,,21.4124,1,name 531,1714,,24.6,1,hair 682,1275,,3.1421,1,bad 1120,1306,,4.1422,1,mud 696,1281,,42.1411,1,bout 647,1072,,44.6,l,hard 672,1580,,47.1428,1,pride 515,1037,,5.l423,1,sod 487,898,,53.1416,1,caught 500,957,,61.0001,1,boy 542,1004,,62.l416,1,goat 496,1021,,63.11101,1,open 439,820,,64.6001,l,board 320,856,,7.1426,1,good 405,954,,72.1422,1,mood 418,966,,74.6003,l,sure 620,1372,,94.6001,1,bird 274,2732,,1.142,1,hid 267,2767,,1l.1421,1,bead 372.2003,,14.00091.1,year 464,2460,,2.1411,1,bet 443,2726,,21.1422,l,made 557,2090,,21.4124,1,name S37,17S6,,24.6,1,hair 788,1570,,3.1421,l,bad 686,1284,,42.14ll,1.bout 746,1612,,44.6,1,hard 664,1270,,47.l428,l,pride 441,859,,5.l423,1,sod 517,781,,53.l416,1,caught 684,944,,6.1422,1,mud S74,1249,,61.0001,1,boy 546,934,,62.1416,1,goat 558.1027,.63.11101,l,open 491,866,,64.6001,l,board 253,908,,7.1426,1,good 225,718,,72.l422,1,mood 406,854,,74.6003,1,sure 73l,1718,,94.6001,1,bird 294,2502,,1.142,1,hid 135 276,2425,,1l.l421,l,bead 356.2266,,14.0009l,l,year 463,1936,,2.1411,l,bet 473,2655,,2l.1422,1,made SS7,2230,,21.4124,1,name 522.1705,,24.6,1,hair 710,1597,,3.l421,l,bad 600,1295,,42.1411,1,bout 800,1433,,44.6,1,hard 666,1387,,47.1428,1,pride 553,1148,,5.l423,1,sod 537,772,,53.l416,1,caught 684,948,,6.1422,l,mud 573,995,,61.0001,l,boy 542.1010,,62.14l6,1,goat 556,1022,,63.lllOl,l,open 455,779,,64.6001,1,board 259,906,,7.1426,1,good 259,759,,72.1422,1,mood 402,844,,74.6003,1,sure 843,1503,,94.6001,l,bird 308.1916,,1.l42,1,hid 292,2216,,1l.1421,1,bead 511.1943,,14.00091,l,year 432,1851,,2.1411,l,bet 397,1864,,21.l422,1,made 535,1963,,21.4124,1,name 505,1604,,24.6,1,hair 609,l645,,3.l421,1,bad 656,1209,,42.1411,1,bout 674,1519,,44.6,1,hard 683.1606,,47.l428,1,pride 492,1148,,5.1423,1,sod 524,982,,53.l4l6,1,caught 782,1253,,6.1422,l,mud 541,942,,61.0001,l,boy 508,950,,62.l416,1,goat 476,965,,63.11101,1,open 512,1041,,64.6001,1,board 382,815,,7.l426,1,good 327,957,,72.1422,l,mood 409,875,,74.6003,1,sure 765,1260,,94.6001,1,bird 346,2240,,1.142,l,hid Zimbabweans All Zimbabweans (2O M&F) 440 37l,2096,,1.l42,1,hid 298,2096,,1l.1421,1,bead 438.1783,,14.00091,1,year 553,2002,,2.1411,l,bet 495,2081,,21.1422,1,made 540,1870,,21.4124,1,name 550,1548,,24.6,1,hair 509,1830,,3.1421,1,bad 637,1309,,42.14ll,1,b0ut 808,1143,,44.6,l,hard 718,1451,,47.l428,l,pride 315,2271,,11.1421,1,bead 530.1915,,14.00091,1,year 428,1802,,2.1411,1,bet 443,2089,,21.1422,1,made 531,1894,,21.4124,1,name 501,1616,,24.6,1,hair 634,1170,,3.1421,1,bad 66l,1220,,42.l411,1,bout 657,1513,,44.6,1,hard 625,1400,,47.1428,1,pride 506,1008,,5.1423,1,sod 457,965,,53.1416,1,caught 647,1148,,6.1422,1,mud 464,901,,61.0001,1,boy 520,953,,62.1416,1,goat 471,986,,63.11101,1,open 474,1121,,64.6001,l,board 353,1034,,7.1426,1,good 303,1054,,72.l422,1,mood 437,1230,,74.6003,1,sure 607.1777,,94.6001,1,bird 3Sl,2374,,1.142,1,hid 318,2302,,11.1421,1,bead 516.1843,.14.00091,1,year 453,1751,,2.1411,1,bet 450,2620,,21.1422,1,made 487,1843,,21.4124,1,name 484,1577,,24.6,1,hair 675,1248,,3.1421,1,bad 649,1190,,42.1411,1,bout 705,1483,,44.6,1,hard 616.1169,,47.1428,1,pride 501,884,,5.l423,1,sod 592,1118,,53.1416,1,caught 708,1502,,6.1422,1,mud 530,917,,61.0001,1,boy 502,932,,62.1416,1,goat 557,935,,63.11101,1,open 506.1229,,64.6001,1,board 326,980,,7.1426,1,good 318,715,,72.l422,1,mood 372,781,,74.6003,1,sure 754,1483,,94.6001,1,bird 539,997,,5.1423,1,sod 597,939,,53.1416,1,Caught 734,1448,,6.l422,1,mud 519,968,,61.0001,l,boy 495,818,,62.1416,1,goat 552,946,,63.11101,1,open 487,923,,64.6001,1,b0ard 400,997,,7.1426,1,good 429,873,,72.1422,1,mood 494,1060,,74.6003,l,sure 560,1995,,94.6001,1,bird 428,2327,,1.142,l,hid 379,2714,,11.l421,1,bead 136 308,2808,,l4.6,l,here 476,2552,,2.l421,l,bed S72,27l3,,21.l422,1,made 528,2544,,24.6,1,hair 718,1203,,3.l421,1,bad 637,1034,,41.0001,l,buy 6Sl,995,,42.l411,l,bout 592,894,,44.6,1,hard 586,989,,S.l424,l,nod 667,1056,,6.1422,1,mud 410,645,,61.0001,1,boy 549,799,,62.1411,l,boat 543,953,,63.0003,1,saw 395,968,,7.l426,1,good 413,876,,72.l422,l,mood 351,960,,74.6003,1,sure 418,902,,74.6003,l,tour 353,894,,82.,1,hue 564,2516,,94.6001,1,bird 398,2580,,1.l42,1,hid 387,2783,,11.1421,1,bead 398,2805,,14.6,l,here 444,2588,,2.1421,l,bed 407,2550,,21.1422,1,made 531,2281,,24.6,1,hair 644,983,,3.1421,1,bad 605.1056,,41.0001,l,buy 605,940,,42.1411,l,bout 663,1006,,44.6,1,hard 53l,874,,5.1424,l,nod 654,991,,6.1422,1,mud 546,874,,61.0001,l,boy 510,969,,62.1411,l,boat 546.1056,,63.0003,l,saw 388,911,,7.1426,l,good 385,697,,72.1422,1,mood 420,1027,,74.6003,l,sure 415,932,,74.6003,1,tour 401,983,,82.,l,hue 517,2340,,94.6001,l,bird 445,2681,,l.l42,1,hid 437,2563,,ll.1421,l,bead 400,1934,,14.6,l,here 430,2288,,2.1421,l,bed 408,2481,,21.1422,l,made 516,2481,,24.6,l,hair 693,946,,3.1421,1,bad 686,972,,41.0001,l,buy 605,988,,42.l4ll,1,bout 603,987,,44.6,1,hard 586,1270,,5.1424,1,nod 868,1206,,6.l422,1,mud 589,1077,,61.0001,1,boy 553,965,,62.l4ll,l,boat SO7,1070,,63.0003,1,saw 341,713,,7.1426,1,good 422,947,,72.1422,l,mood 430,951,,74.6003,1,sure 438,876,,74.6003,l,tour 324,1031,,82.,1,hue 467.2407,,94.6001,1,bird 395,2474,,1.l42,1,hid 372,2906,,11.1421,1,bead 342,2847,,l4.6,1,here 4S4,2511,,2.1421,1,bed 496,2585,,21.1422,1,made 491,2608,,24.6,1,hair 655,1013,,3.l421,1,bad 737.1117,,41.0001,1,buy 617,993,,42.1411,1,bout 640,1043,,44.6,1,hard 573,939,,5.1424,1,nod 722,1265,,6.1422,1,mud 485,916,,61.0001,1,boy 521,947,,62.l411,l,boat 521,954,,63.0003,1,saw 320,805,,7.1426,l,good 380,1088,,72.1422,1,mood 373,975,,74.6003,1,tour 358,916,,74.6003,1,sure 358,1334,,82.,1,hue 513,1886,,94.6001,l,bird 404,2661,,1.l42,1,hid 404,2555,,11.1421,1,bead 351,2646,,14.6,1,here 526,2173,,2.1421,1,bed 549,2067,,21.1422,1,made 472,2578,,24.6,1,hair 643,1053,,3.1421,1,bad 682,976,,41.0001,1,buy 633,984,,42.1411,1,bout 580,1044,,44.6,1,hard S49,1260,,5.1424,l,nod 686,1213,,6.1422,1,mud 442,762,,61.0001,1,boy 549,999,,62.1411,1.boat 541,1042,,63.0003,1,saw 348,828,,7.1426,1,good 395,1053,,72.1422,l,mood 401,960,,74.6003,1,sure 450,1053,,74.6003,l,tour 396,923,,82.,1,hue 556,1891,,94.6001,1,bird 402,2384,,1.142,1,hid 401,2816,,11.l421,1,bead 402.2504,,l4.6,1,here 521.2303,,2.1421,1,bed 551,2511,,21.1422,l,made 539,2616,,24.6,1,hair 618,894,,3.1421,1,bad 618,976,,41.0001,1,buy 64l,993,,42.l411,1,bout 700,1028,,44.6,1,hard 588,953,,5.1424,1,nod 886,1422,,6.l422,l,mud 566,723,,61.0001,1,boy 676,1247,,42.1411,1,b0ut 694,953,,44.6,l,hard S48,993,,5.l424,1,n0d 865,1264,,6.1422,1,mud 513,924,,62.l4ll,l,b0at 469,990,,63.0003,l,saw 365,857,,7.l426,1,g00d 402,865,,72.l422,l,mood 343,782,,74.6003,1,sure 469,857,,74.6003,1,tour 432,990,,82.,l,hue 483,1722,,94.6001,l,bird 301,243S,,l.l42,l,hid 306,2675,,1l.l421,1,bead 360,2308,,l4.6,l,here 511,2352,,2.1421,l,bed 517,2617,,21.1422,1,made 493,2454,,24.6,l,hair 610,911,,3.1421,l,bad 595,955,,41.0001,l,buy 659,1084,,42.14ll,1,bout 793,1290,,44.6,1.hard 396,889,,5.l424,l,nod 644,1230,,6.l422,l,mud 580,1014,,6l.0001,1,boy 506,896,,62.l4ll,1,boat 521,904,,63.0003,l,saw 322,881,,7.1426,l,good 388,728,,72.1422,l,mood 407,900,,74.6003,l,sure 447,874,,74.6003,1,tour 327,2419,,82.,l,hue 435,1896,,94.6001,l,bird 412,2605,,l.l42,1,hid 381,2480,,11.l421,1,bead 358,2457,,14.6,1,here 450,2473,,2.l421,1,bed 482,2659,,21.1422,1,made 428,2551,,24.6,1,hair 664,1057,,3.l421,l,bad 637,995,,41.0001,l,buy 640,1052,,42.l4ll,1,bout 69l,972,,44.6,1,hard 428,1150,,5.1424,l,nod 660,1136,,6.l422,l,mud 524,941,,61.0001,l,boy 521.1019,,62.l411,1,boat 521.1019,,63.0003,1,saw 365,863,,7.1426,l,good 346,972,,72.1422,l,mood 365,762,,74.6003,l,sure 551,948,,74.6003,l,tour 322,972,,82.,l,hue 544,1921,,94.6001,1,bird 398,2489,,1.l42,1,hid 481,2528,,1l.l421,1,bead 382,2558,,14.6,l,here 436,2437,,2.1421,l,bed 413,2482,,21.l422,1,made 444,2415,,24.6,l,hair 614,962,,3.1421,l,bad 616,993,,41.0001,1,buy 533,985,,61.0001,l,boy 526,910,,62.1411,1,boat 556,1218,,63.0003,1,saw 330,932,,7.1426,l,good 360,752,,72.1422,l,mood 451,782,,74.6003,1,sure 496,1030,,74.6003,1,tour 360,894,,82.,1,hue 444,1956,,94.6001,1,bird 411,2460,,l.l42,1,hid 395.2857,,1l.1421,1,bead 426,2625,,14.6,1,here 433,2736,,2.1421,1,bed 414,2550,,21.1422,1,made 425,2594,,24.6,1,hair 684,950,,3.1421,1,bad 687,972,,41.0001.1,buy 657,1076,,42.14ll,1,bout 698,912,,44.6,l,hard 511,1203,,S.l424,1,nod 680,1128,,6.1422,1,mud 523,1047,,61.0001,1,boy 530,920,,62.1411,1,boat 486,1092,,63.0003,1,saw 381,935,,7.1426,1,good 366,1064,,72.1422,1,mood 386.778,,74.6003,1,sure 448,964,,74.6003,1,tour 403.1039,,82.,1,hue 486,1869,,94.6001,1,bird 416,2207,,1.l42,1,hid 364,2207,,11.l421,l,bead 459,2041,,14.00091,l,year 527,1762,,2.1411,1,bet 610,2014,,21.1422,1,made 394,1869,,21.4124,1,name 557.1957,,24.6,1,hair 639.1747,,3.l421,1,bad 647,1318,,42.1411,l,bout 698,1159,,44.6,l,hard 706,1333,,47.1428,l,pride 505,996.,5.1423,l,sod 658,1086,,53.1416,1,caught 706,1086,,6.1422,1,mud 504,1015,,61.0001,1,boy 500,822,,62.1416,1,goat 572.1033,,63.11101,l,open 512,899,,64.6001,1,board 394,959,,7.1426,1,good 378,708,,72.1422,1,mood 533,1120,,74.6003,l,sure 520,1635,,94.6001,1,bird 357,2182,,1.142,1,hid 330,2645,,11.1421,l,bead 659,1174,,44.6,l,hard 647,1389,,47.l428,1,pride 614,1158,,S.1423,1,sod 515,1128,,53.l4l6,l,caught 630,1367,,6.1422,l,mud 59l,952,,6l.0001,l,boy 504,860,,62.l4l6,l,goat 498,1013,,63.lllOl,l,open 491.1059,,64.6001,1,board 373,1097,,7.l426,l,good 360,767,,72.l422,l,mood 447,1204,,74.6003,1,sure 484,1419,,94.6001,1,bird 364,2200,,l.l42,1,hid 363.2177,,1l.1421,l,bead 489.1985,,14.00091,l,year 486,1168,,2.l4ll,l,bet 553.2038,,21.1422,1,made 445,1948,,21.4124,l,name S97,l789,,24.6,1,hair 697,1517,,3.l421,1,bad 666,1353,,42.14ll,l,bout 804,1388,,44.6,l,hard 644.1371,,47.1428,l,pride 515,986,,5.l423,l,sod 682.1039,,53.l416,l,caught 811,1381,,6.1422,l,mud 584,1066,,6l.0001,1,boy 442,834,,62.14l6,1,goat 538,986,,63.11101,1,open 543,1039,,64.6001,1,board 360,1039,,7.l426,1,good 337,756,,72.l422,1,mood 465,1013,,74.6003,1,sure 553,1578,,94.6001,l,bird 349,2078,,1.142,l,hid 337,2234,,ll.1421,1,bead 469,2258,,l4.00091,1,year 418,199l,,2.1411,1,bet 416.2227,,21.l422,1,made 470,1900,,21.4124,1,name 538,2006,,24.6,1,hair 580,1812,,3.1421,1,bad 667,1380,,42.1411,1,bout 721,1223,,44.6,1,hard 635,1461,,47.l428,1,pride 572,1168,,5.1423,1,sod 563,898,,53.l4l6,l,caught 698,1529,,6.l422,l,mud 556,1115,,61.0001,1,boy 466,849,,62.1416,1,goat 454,898,,63.11101,1,open 507,917,,64.6001,1,board 4OS,1024,,7.1426,1,good 410,784,,72.1422,1,mood 510,1042,,74.6003,1,sure 470,1949,,94.6001,1,bird APPENDIX C 140 APPENDIX C An Example of a Plotnik Vowel Chart 3000 2:300 2600 2100 22100 2900 ratio 1600 1100 12.00 1000 090 200 300_ . lI) bead I 0 good h'd u o" 400. ‘ ° mod 0 gear . e 0 . 7 fi 500_ “a" sure boll sod goat 0 .,. board 600- . k sod caught J E bout 700‘ Amud '17 a pride 800- hard : I Jones,33,Formal,Rural,Zimbabwe 141 APPENDIX D 142 Gender: Age: Mother Tongue: Please listen carefully to the following pairs of words. For each pair indicate whether the two words you hear are the same or different by marking (‘1 ) appropriately. For example QUESTIONNAIRE USED TO ELICIT VOWEL PERCEPTIONS APPENDIX D if words in pair 1 sound the same to you, mark: Pair 1: (a) Same ‘1 What we mean by ‘same’ or ‘different’ is :- is the speaker pronouncing the same words in each pair or is he pronouncing different words? You are simply judging the speaker’s (b) different pronunciation and your judgement is correct. Pair 1 : Pmr2: Pair 3: Pmr4: Pair 5: Pair 6: Pair 7: Pair 8: Pair 9: Pair 10: Pair 11: Pair 12: Pair 13: Pair 14: Pair 15: Pair 16: Pair 17: Pair 18: Pair [9: Pair 20: Pair 21: Pair 22: Pair 23: Pair 24: Pair 25: Pair 26: (a) Same (a) Same (a) Same (a) Same (a) Same (a) Same (a) Same (a) Same (a) Same (b) different (b) different (b) different (b) different (b) different (b) different (b) different (b) different (b) different (a) Same (a) Same (a) Same (a) Same (a) Same (a) Same (a) Same (a) Same (a) Same (a) Same (a) Same (a) Same (a) Same (a) Same (a) Same (a) Same (a) Same (b) different (b) different (b) different (b) different (b) different (b) different (b) different (b) different (b) different (b) different (b) different (b) different (b) different (b) different (b) different (b) different (b) different 143 Pair 27: (a) Same (b) different Pair 28: (3) Same (b) different Pair 29: (a) Same (b) different Pair 30: (a) Same (b) different Pair 31: (a) Same (b) different Pair 32: (a) Same (b) different Pair 33: (a) Same (b) different Pair 34: (a) Same (b) different Pair 35: (a) Same (b) different Pair 36: (a) Same (b) different Pair 37: (a) Same (b) different Pair 38 : (a) Same (b) different Pair 39: (a) Same (b) different Pair 40: (a) Same (b) different Pair 41: (a) Same (b) different Pair 42: (a) Same (b) different Pair 43: (a) Same (b) different Pair 44: (a) Same (b) different Pair 45: (a) Same (b) different Pair 46: (a) Same (b) different Pair 47: (a) Same (b) different Pair 48: (a) Same (b) different Pair 49: (a) Same (b) different Pair 50: (a) Same (b) different Pair 51: (a) Same (b) different Pair 52: (a) Same (b) different Pair 53: (a) Same (b) different Pair 54: (a) Same (b) different Pair 55: (a) Same (b) different Pair 56: (a) Same (b) different Pair 57: (a) Same (b) different Pair 58: (a) Same (b) different Please continue with section B B. Please listen carefully to the following pairs of words and write down each word that the speaker pronounced. This should work like a dictation exercise. Once again whatever you hear is correct, it is an individual judgement and that is all that counts. So don’t hesitate to write exactly what you hear. 144 Pair 1: (a) Pair 2: (a) Pair 3: (a) Pair 4: (a) Pair 5: (a) Pair 6: (a) Pair 7: (a) Pair 8: (a) Pair 9: (a) Fair 10: (a) (b) (b) (b) (b) (b) (b) (b) (b) (b) (b) Pair 11: (a) (b) Pair 12: (a) (b) Pair 13: (a) (b) Pair 14: (a) (b) Pair 15: (a) (b) Pair 16: (a) (b) Pair 17: (a) (b) Pair 18: (a) (b) Pair 19: (a) Pair 20: (a) (b) (b) Pmr2h (a) (b) Fair 22: (a) (b) Pair 23: (a) (b) Pair 24: (a) Pair 25: (a) Pair 26: (a) Pair 27: (a) Pair 28: (a) Pair29: (a) Pair 30: (a) Pair 31: (a) Pair 32: (a) Pair 33: (a) Pair 34: (a) Pair 35: (a) Pair 36: (a) Pair 37: (a) Pair 38: (a) Pair 39: (a) Pair 40: (a) Pair 41: (a) Pair 42: (a) Pair 43: (a) Pair 44: (a) Pair 45: (a) Pair 46: (a) 145 (b) (b) (b) (b) (b) (b) (b) (b) (b) (b) (b) (b) (b) (b) (b) (b) (b) (b) (b) (b) (b) (b) (b) Pair 47: (a) Pair 48: (a) Pair 49: (a) Pair 50: (a) Pair 51: (a) Pair 52: (a) (b) (b) (b) (b) (b) (b) Pair 53: (a) Pair 54: (a) Pair 55: (a) Pair 56: (a) Pair 57: (a) Pair 58: (a) Thank you very much for your participation. 146 (b) (b) (b) (b) (b) (b) APPENDIX E 147 (a) Stimulus [a] APPENDIX E IDENTIFICATION TASK RESULTS Stimuli minimal pairs Gh'. Respondents' Zim. Respondents' Ken. Respondents' words played to perception of stimuli perception of stimuli perception of stimuli respondents vowels (in %) vowels (in %) vowels (in %) a ~ a a ~ a 23-33 a ~ a 47-84 a ~ a 46-50 bat/bat A ~ A 75-63 A ~ A 50-16 A ~ A 50-41 a~ A a~A 79-53 a~A 85-41 a~A 67-42 mad/mud A ~ a 15-36 A ~ 3 10-47 A ~ a 26-51 a~a a~a 74-50 a~a 81-88 a~a 56—28 a ~a 15-41 (1 ~21 19-09 a:~a 21-49 had/hard 3 ~ 3 19-16 a~ai a~ai 65-12 a~ai 84-16 a~ai 70-41 au ~ a 17-58 A ~ a 06-72 A ~ a 22-37 dam/dime a~au A~A 74.43 a~a 50-53 a~au 51-31 a~a 29-35 A~A 47-16 A~a 42-20 bat/I301“ au=o au=0 ai ~ 31 06'18 a~D a~D 73-12 a~D 84-65 a~D' 73-00 cat/cor a ~ 0 73-54 A ~ A 10-19 A ~ 15-63 A~A 27-27 a:~a 06-17 0: ~ 0 3: ~ 0 70-28 0: ~ 0 98-56 0: ~ 0 61-41 caught/cot A ~ a 23-47 A ~ 0 02-34 on ~ 0: 24.47 0 ~ 0_ 05-16 3 ~ 3 Q ~ (F. 92-96 g: ~ a: 97-91 Q. ~ (B 98-93 walk/work c~ua c~ua 54-20 c~ua 100-100 c~ua 15-30 law/lure 0 ~ 0 37-59 0 ~ 0 84-43 8 ~ 9 01-18 ' Gh denotes Ghanaian, Zim , Zimbabwean , and Ken. Kenyan 148 (c) Stimulus [u] Stimuli minimal pairs words played to Gh’s respondents perception of stimuli vowels (in %) Zim’s respondents perception of stimuli vowels (in %) Ken’s respondents perception of stimuli vowels (in %) u ~ u u ~ u 96-96 u ~ u 100-73 u ~ u 75-74 good/good u ~ 0 -do- 27 on ~ ou 15-15 u ~ U u ~ U 58-52 u ~ 6 94-76 11 ~ u 86-30 wow/“06d U ~ “ 3348 ~ 0 03-27 u ~ u 1445 u ~ju u ~ju 64-91 u ~ju 94-76 u ~ju 83-83 who/hue ju ~u 36-09 U A. u 03-15 u ~ ou u ~ ou 66-61 u ~ ou 100-49 11 ~ ou 79-26 boot/boat A ~ A 25-18 u ~ 0 -do- 35 A ~ 0 10-50 U ~ U U ~ U 96-98 U ~ U 100-100 U ~ U 93-93 book/book U ~ ou U ~ ou 96-49 . U ~ on 98-33 U ~ on 92-35 cook/coke u ~ U 0347 u ~ 0 02-67 0 ~ 3: 08-45 ((1) Stimulus [I] Stimuli minimal pairs Gh’s respondents Zim’s respondents Ken’s respondents words played to perception of stimuli perception of stimuli perception of stimuli respondents vowels (in %) vowels (in %) vowels (in %) I ~ I I ~ I 60-45 I ~ I 39-30 I ~ I 25-20 Hid/hid 1 ~ 1 22-14 1 ~i 39-42 6 ~ 8 63-60 1~ 8 1~ 8 68-36 I~ 8 61-61 I~ 8 52-63 Bit/bet g... A 30-32 i ~ a 36—16 i ~I 41-17 I ~ 3 I ~ 3 83-66 I ~ 3 39-63 I ~ 3 40-25 Bid/bird I~ 8 19-34 i ~ 6 48-20 1 ~ a 25-66 8 ~ A 21-06 149 I ~ ia I ~ ia 50-36 I ~ ia 49-61 I ~ ia 35-80 Bid-beard 1 ~ i 29-19 i ~i 46-29 i ~ i 30-14 e ~a 16-04 i~I i~I 68-48 i~I 41-24 i~I 75-21 beat/bit I ~ i 32-38 I ~ i 59-41 I ~ i 21- 28 i ~8 -do- -36 I ~ 8 ~do-—49 (e) Stimulus [e] Stimuli minimal pairs Gh’s respondents Zim’s respondents Ken’s respondents words played to perception of stimuli perception of stimuli perception of stimuli respondents vowels (in %) vowels (in %) vowels (in %) e ~ e e ~ e 96-95 e ~ e 97-96 e ~ e 48-49 made/made ai ~ ai 43-49 e~8 e~8 83-13 e~8 73-46 e~ 8 31-70 wade/wed .. 3 53 ~ 9 -37 ai 77-13 3 19 a1 06 e ~ 3 e ~ 3 85-78 e ~ 3 83-06 e ~ 3 23-79 wade/word 3 ... 8 10-09 ~ a: -94 ai ~ a: 77-13 ~ e -08 e ~ a e ~ a 36-34 e ~ a 84-84 e ~ a 56-52 bait/bat 8 ~ A 20—61 ~ A -13 ai ~ A 3344 e~ia e~ia 37-61 e~ia 55-100 e~ia 45-100 bay/beer 8~ea 26-34 ia ~ ia 23- -do- ai ~ la 26- -do- 3 ~ ia 16- -do e ~ ea c ~ ea 53-39 e ~ ea 83-38 e ~ ea 57-57 hay/hair a ~ 3 21-24 ~ e -25 ai ~ 3 29-18 a: ~ 3 13-22 i~e i~e 64-43 i~e 43-59 i~e 57-14 beat/bait 1~A 31-17 I~ai 47-13 a~a 60-60 1 ~ a -do- -13 (f) Stimulus [8] 150 Stimuli minimal pairs Gh. respondents Zim. respondents Ken. respondents words played to perception of stimuli perception of stimuli perception of stimuli respondents vowels (in %) vowels (in %) vowels (in %) 8~8 8~8 51-42 8~8 55-58 8~8 71-60 bet/bet a ~ 3 40-40 a ~ a 22-27 8 ~ e 13-18 e~e 15-10 a~a 13-11 8~3 8~3 31-38 8~3 55-62 8~3 56-16 bed/bird 3 ~ a 31-27 3 ~ a 15-21 3 ~ a 36-73 a ~ ea 20-24 8~a 8~a 26-42 8~a 44-84 8~a 72-83 bed/bad 3 ~ A 36-42 a~ A 34-16 3 ~ 3 28-07 A ~ a: 28-08 e ~ 22- i~8 i~8 100-22 i~e 98-67 i~8 93-65 weed/wed .- 3 - 61 ia ~o 02-19 ai ~ ai 07-19 Stimulus [3] Stimuli minimal pairs Gh. respondents Zim. respondents Ken. respondents words played to perception of stimuli perception of stimuli perception of stimuli respondents vowels (in %) vowels (in %) vowels (in %) 3 ~ 3 3 ~ 3 68-57 3 ~ 3 73-50 3 ~ 3 32-28 bird/bird a ~ 8 13-17 a ~ a 23-27 a ~ a 60-60 8~a 09-11 8 --—l4 3 ~ 8 3 ~ a 45-62 3 ~ 3 45-91 3 ~ a 27-73 bird/bad g .. A 22-31 a ~ 8 27-03 a ~ a: 56—15 A ~g 18-04 e~A 21-03 a: ~A 10-12 3 ~ A 3 ~ A 40-33 3 ~ A 42-16 3 ~ A 46-15 bird/bud 8 ~ a 22-44 a ~ a: 35-61 a ~ a 54-78 a~3 20-11 0 ~0 10-13 3 ~ 0 3 ~ 0 84-84 3 ~ 0 53-65 3 ~ 0 41-63 heard/hoed a ~ 3 09-05 a: ~ 8 38-24 a: ~ 0: 32-20 3 ~ an 17-17 3 ~ 0: 3 ~ 0: 50-32 3 ~ 3: 34-84 3 ~ 0: 39-61 a~oi 18-45 oz~oi 34-16 a~o 60-21 bird/board 8 ~ 3 16-11 a ~ 22 151 8~3 8~3 31-38 8~3 55-62 8~3 56-16 bed/bird 3 ~ a 31-27 3 ~ a 15-21 a ~ a 36-73 a ~ ea 20-24 I ~ 3 I ~ 3 83-66 I ~ 3 39-63 I ~ 3 40-25 Bid/bird I~8 19-34 i~e 48-20 i~a 25-66 8 ~ A 21-06 Stimulus [A] Stimuli minimal pairs Gh. respondents Zim. respondents Ken. respondents words played to perception of stimuli perception of stimuli perception of stimuli respondents vowels (in %) vowels (in %) vowels (in %) A~A 0~A 08-04 A~A 13-13 A~A 13-13 rug/rug 0 ~ 0 83-83 3 ~ 0 87-87 3 ~ 0 54-50 3 ~ 3 13-16 A ~a A~a 48-73 A~a 52-91 A~a 57-60 mud/mad a~A 41-25 a~e 31-07 a~A 45-38 A ~ 3 A ~ 3 07-78 33 ~ 81 49-57 A ~ 3 24-48 hut/hurt 3 ~ 3 68-11 0 ~ 0 43'2” 3 ~ A 62-26 e~A 09-11 a~a 22-26 A ~ 8 A ~ 8 92-37 A ~ 8 06-57 A ~ 8 57-50 land/lend 3 ~ A 04 -37 a ~ a 94-40 a ~ a 33-35 i ~ i 02-14 A~a A~a 04-76 A~a 4-42 A~a 20-57 gut/got 0 ~ ou 62-13 3 ~ ou 79-45 3 ~ ou 63-21 g ~3 22-07 ou~e 21-12 A~o A~o 41-68 A~o 18-64 A~o 34-20 mud/mode a ~ A 24-12 a ~ a: 36-21 a ~ on 40-48 0~3 45-15 ou~a 14-10 A~a: A~a: 45-31 o~oz 59-78 A~a: 15-40 bud/board 0 ~ oi 24-36 a ~0u 19-12 a ~ 0 30-23 3 ~23 10-26 3 ~o 23-17 3~A 3~A 40-33 3~A 42-16 3~A 46-15 bird/bud 8 ~ a 22-44 a ~ a: 35—61 3 ~ a 54-78 a~3 20-11 0 ~0 10-13 a ~ A a ~A 79-53 a ~ A 85-41 a ~ A 67-42 mad/mud A ~ a 15-36 A ~ a 10-47 A ~ a 26-51 152 3 ~ A Bought/but 3~A 46-80 ou~a 30-11 A~o_ 15-07 0 ~A 73-42 ou~a 27- 35 0~A 61-59 ou~o36-3O au~a 16-23 Stimulus [C1] Stimuli minimal pairs Gh. respondents Zim. respondents Ken. respondents words played to perception of stimuli perception of stimuli perception of stimuli respondents vowels (in %) vowels (in %) vowels (in %) 0 ~ 0 0 ~ 0 50-38 0 ~ 0 57-55 0 ~ 0 38-35 hot/hot a ~ a 15-22 a ~ a 14-39 a ~ a 24-24 3 ~3 15-22 3 ~3 24-24 0 ~ au 0 ~ au 77-63 0 ~ au 58-31 0 ~ au 75-35 got/gout 3 ~ 0 12-21 3 ~ 2 41- 50 011 ~ on 25-38 ou ~ 0 07-16 0 ~32 a~a: 14-71 a~a: 6-85 a~a: 15-70 con/caught 0 ~ ou 52-17 3 ~ A 94-06 0 ~ ou 61-19 A ~A 29-14 0 ~ 01 0 ~ oi 11-56 0 ~ oi 4-24 0 ~ oi 38-27 sod/soy 0 ~ 0: 60-29 3 ~ 3: 88-70 0 ~ 3: 23-33 0: ~o 17-21 0 ~ ua Cl ~ ua 17-95 0 ~ ua 37-50 0 ~ ua 14-91 cod/cure A ~ 0 37-05 0 ~ ju 60-47 a: ~.o 37-08 3 ... ua 10_-d0_ a: ~ ua 3l--do- i ~ ia i ~ ia 74-100 i ~ ia 79-76 i ~ ia 68-100 he/here ia ~ ia 17- -do- ia ~ i 21-24 ia ~ ia 32- -do- 1 ~ ju 1 ~ ju 85-73 i~ju 94-58 i~ju 82-69 he/hue ju ~i 15-17 ia ~i 06-39 ia ~i 09-32 153 0 ~ oi 0 ~ oi 11-56 0 ~ oi 4-24 0 ~ oi 38-27 sod/80y 0 ~ 3: 60-29 0 ~ 3: 88-70 3 ~ 3: 23-33 3: ~ 0 17-21 0 ~ ua 0 ~ ua 17-95 0 ~ ua 37-50 0 ~ ua 14-91 cod/cure A .... 0 37-05 3 ~ ju 60.47 0: ~ 0 37-08 0 ~ ua lO--dO- 3: ~ 113 31'-dO- 0 ~ 30 0 ~ au 77-63 0 ~ an 58-31 0 ~ au 75-35 got/gout 0 ~ 6 12-21 3 ~ 3 41- 50 011 ~ on 25-38 ou ~ 0 07-16 154 APPENDIX F 155 APPENDIX F DISCRIMINATION RESULTS Table 1- Results of Minimal Pair Tests administered to Kenyan, Ghanaian and Malawian University students. Ghanaian Zimbabwean Kenyan Discrimination Discrimination Discrimination Vowel pairs S D S D S D tested "/0 "/o % % % % I ~ ia 50 50 4O 60 35 65 Bid/beard 8 ~ 8 68 32 34 66 49 51 made/ made e ~ 8 26 74 13 87 51 49 wade/wed 3 ~ A 46 54 50 50 57 43 bird/bud 21 ~ 3 88 12 48 52 78 22 bat/bat a -. A 24 76 37 67 55 45 mad/mud a ~ au 46 54 27 73 57 43 hat/bout A - A 96 4 57 43 78 22 rug/rug A .- 3 64 36 53 47 49 51 hut/hurt - A - a so so 35 65 67 33 gut/got A .- o 26 74 22 78 47 53 mud/mode a .-. a 52 48 50 50 57 43 hot/hot a - 0,, 32 68 33 67 57 43 got/goat a - a; 36 64 37 63 53 47 cot/caught 3 ~ 3 66 34 52 48 63 37 call/call 3 ~ (Ju 12 88 8 92 57 43 bought/bout 156 3; ~ g 20 80 42 58 39 61 caught/cot 3 - ua 42 58 so 50 47 53 law/lure u ~ u 74 26 45 55 65 35 good/good u - U 44 56 25 75 37 63 wood/wooed U - DU 52 48 45 55 49 51 cook/coke 157 REFERENCES 158 REFERENCES Adegbija, BE. 1994. Language Attitudes in Sub-Saharan Africa. A Sociolinguistic Overview. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters Ltd. Anderson, Stephen. 1985. Phonology in the Twentieth Century. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. 7‘ Angogo, R. and Hancock, 1. 1980. English in Africa: Emerging Standards or Diverging Regionalisms? English World- Wide: A Journal of Varieties of English 1(1): 67—96. PBailey, L, and Gorlach, M. (eds) 1982. English as a World Language. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press. Baker, S. 1966. The Australian Language. Sydney: Currawong. It Bokamba, E. 1981. The Africanization of English. In Kachru, B.B.(ed.).The Other Tongue. English Across Cultures. Urban: Illinois University Press, 77—98. \[ Angogo, R. & 1. Hancock. 1980. “ English in Africa: Emerging standards or diverging regionalisms?”. English World- Wide] :6 7-96. “f Bailey, L. and Gorlach, M. (eds.) 1982. English as a World Language. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press. Banda, Felix. 1996. “ The scope and characterization of African English: Some sociolinguistic considerations”. English World- Wide 17:1 63-75. Best, C.T., McRoberts, G.W., Sithole, N.M. (1988) Examination of perceptual reorganization for nonnative speech contrasts: Zulu click discrimination by En glish-speaking adults and infants. Journal of Experimantal Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. Vol. 14, No. 3, 345-360 \L Bokamba, E. 1981. "The Africanization of English". In Kachru, ed. The Other Tongue: English Across Cultures. Urbana: Illinois University Press, 77-98 Dipaolo, Marianna. 1988. Pronunciation and categorization in sound change. K. Ferrara, et a1(eds.), Linguistic Change and Contact: NWAV-XVI. Austin: University of 159 Texas, 84-92. Dwyer, David J. 1969. An introduction to West African Pidgin English. African Studies Center, Michigan State University Flege, James E., and E. Frieda. 1997. Amount of native-language (L1) use affects the pronunciation of an L2. Journal of Phonetics 25, 169-186. Flege, James E., and S. Jang. 1997. Effects of experience on non-native speakers’ production and perception of English vowels. Journal of Phonetics 25, 437-470 Gimson, AC. 1980. Introduction to the Pronunciation of English.London: Longman. Gorlach, M. 1987. "English as a world language: the state of art". English World- Wide 9: 1-32. 1991. Englishes: Studies in Varieties of English 1984-88. Amsterdam: Benjamins. 1995. More Englishes: New Studies in Varieties of English, 1988-94. Amsterdam:Benj amins Hancock, 1. F. and Angogo, R. 1982. English in East Africa. In Bailey, R. and Gorlach, M. (eds.), English as a World Language. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press, 306-23. Harms, LS. 1961. Listener’s judgements of status cues in speech. Quarterly Journal of Speech, 47,164-68. Heine, B. 1970. Status and Use of African Lingua F rancas. Munich: WTV. Herbert, R.K.(ed) 1992. Language and Society in Africa. The Theory and Practice of Sociolinguistics. Cape Town. Witwatersrand University Press. Hinnebusch, T. 1989. “Bantu” in J. Bender-Samuel (ed.). The Niger—Congo Languages. Lanham,MD.: UPA 160 Ingram, John C., and S. Park. 1997. Cross-language vowel perception and production by Japanese and Korean learners of English. Journal of Phonetics 25, 343-370 Kachru, B.B.(ed) 1982.The Other Tongue: English Across Cultures. Urbana: University of Illinois Press. Lambert, W.E. et a1 Evaluational reaction of Jewish and Arab adolescents to dialect and language variations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1965, 2, 84 - 90. 1960. Evaluational reaction to spoken languageJournal of Abnormal Social Psychology, 60: 44-51. Lanham, L.W. 1982. English in South Africa. In Bailey, R. and Gorlach, M. (eds). English as a World Language. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press, 353-83. Jibril, Munzali. 1986. “Sociolinguistic variation in Nigerian English”. English World- Wide 7:47-75 Jusczyk, Peter. 1986. Speech perception. In Kenneth Boff, Lloyd Kaufman, James Thomas (eds.) Handbook of Perception and Human Perception: Volume II Cognitive Processes and Performance.New York: John Wiley Publications. Kenstowiscz, M. 1994. Generative Phonology. Cambridge, Ma. Blackwell Kuhl, Patricia. 1993. Early linguistic experience and phonetic perception: implications for theories of developmental speech perception. Journal of Phonetics, 21, 125- 139. Labov, Williams. 1966. The Social Stratification of English in New York City. Washington DC: Center for Applied Linguistics. 1972. Sociolinguistic Patterns. Philadeiphia: University of Pennyslavania Press. 1984. Field methods of the project on linguistic change and variation. In John Bauch and Joel sherzer (eds), Language in Use: Readings in 161 I ' . pus-"mm? é Sociolinguistics. New Jersey: Prentice-Hall. 1994. Principles of Linguistic Change. Cambridge, MA: Blackwell Ladefoged, P. & Maddieson, I. 1990. Vowels of the world’s languages in Journal of Phonetics 18, 93-122 Lieberman, A.M. & Mattiningly, LG. 1985. The motor theory of speech perception revised, Cognition, 21, 1-36. Lieberman, P. & Blumstein, S. 1993. Speech Physiology, Speech Perception, and Acoustic PhoneticsNew York: Cambridge University Press. Mackay, D.G. et a1. 1987. Relationships and modules within language perception and production: An introduction. In Alan Allport, Donald Mackay, Wolfgang Prinz, Eckart Scheerer (eds.) Language Perception and Production: Relationships Between Listening, peaking, Reading and Writing. Academic Press: London. Obiechina, Emmanuel. 1974. " Variety differentiation in English usage". Journal of Nigerian Studies Association Vol. 6, no.1 74-94 Peterson, G., and H. Barney. 1952. Control methods used in a study of vowels. Journal of Acoustical Society of America 24. 174-84. Preston, Dennis R.(ed) 1993. American Dialect Research. Philadelphia: John Benjamins publishing company. 1988. Methods in the study of dialect perception. In Alan Thomas (ed.), Methods in dialectology. Clevedon, Avon and Philadelphia: Multilingual Matters, 373-95 1989. Sociolinguistics and Second Language Acquisition. New York: Blackwell. 1999. Folk metalanguage: Michigan State University (unpublished) 1999. The northern cities chain shift in your mind. Michigan State University (unpublished) 162 Pride, J. (ed.). 1982. new Englishes. Rowley, Mass.: Newbury House Schmied, J. 1991. English in Africa: An Introduction. London: Longman. 1990. " Language use, attitudes, performance and sociolinguistic background: A study of English in Kenya, Tanzania, and Zambia". English World-Wide 1 1:2 217-238 Schneider, E. (ed.) 1997. Englishes Around the World: Studies in Honour of Manfred Gorlach. Vol. 1&2. Amsterdam: Benjamins. Sridhar, Kama] K. 1985.Sociolinguistic theory and non-native varieties of English, Lingua 68, 39-58. Strevens, Peter. 1971. World English and the world's Englishes - or whose language is it anyway? Journal of the Royal Society of Art 130: 418-31. Sure, K. 1991. Language functions and Language attitudes in Kenya.English World- Wide 12 (2), 245-60. Todd, L. & W.R. O’Donnell .1980. Variety in Contemporary English. London: G. Allen Todd, L. 1982. Cameroon. Heidelberg: Gross 1984 Modern Englishes: Pidgins and Creoles. Oxford: Blackwell & 1. Hancock .1987. International English Usage. New York: NYUP Thiong’o, Ngugi 1984. Decolonizing the Mind: The Politics of Language in African Literature. Nairobi: Heinneman. Welmers, E. 1973. African Languages Structures. Berkley: University of California Press. Werker, J. & Polka, L. 1993. Developmental changes in speech perception: New challenges and directions. Journal of Phonetics, 21, 83-101. Werker, J. &Tees, R. 1984 Cross-language speech perception: evidence for perceptual reorganization during the first year of life, Infant Behavior and Development, &, 49-63. Westerman, D.& Ward, 1. 1990. Practical Phonetics for Students of African Languages. London: Kegan Paul Intern. Ltd. 163 Yang, B.G. 1996. A comparative study of American English and Korean vowels. Journal of Phonetics 24,245-261) 164 11