37:25“. . 1.: ; .v. : «Sim .wmwkmrfl . . 2...»? r Y. «3?... .‘ $i+. . . I . .. . . .. .7 .. _ “.31.“. in“? . . .fizéxév... . . . , 53 La .2 . ‘ 3:. Fun? 2 .. .. . , .. ‘135 n V 4,. :fiafi. . ‘ , _ ‘ . wwwagm Ti. 9 . Emma . : 5 . . . . . :4 “War. , ‘ :56 d. , x . . , . . .3! 4. . .HLJ: ‘ 1 ‘ , . ‘ . , . $5.5...“ d...” ._ . . V V . .. . . . 43.. 9.1:“qu v: ~13: 8...... ~ by . . .. .3. 79...? f .5}? a . 9... .. «I 1. , .3}. $ 1.1.1.2.? a . .11... . ‘ , . . . . ‘ urine}. «.151 , ‘ . , , . q ..s..\... )3" THESIS l 32 V? C7 This is to certify that the dissertation entitled Parent Management Training (PMT) for Children with Disruptive Behavior Disorders: Motivational Aspects presented by Anne Marie Caruso has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for Ph .D . degree in School Psychology Hm F (lemme. gajor professor Date 12/08/00 MS U is an Affirmative Action/Equal Opportunity Institution 0-12771 LEBRARY Michigan State University PLACE IN RETURN BOX to remove this checkout from your record. TO AVOID FINES return on or before date due. MAY BE RECALLED with earlier due date if requested. DATE n21 Q DEER DATE DUE DATE DUE OCT17 5-wa- v RU 205 11/00 chIRC/DatoDmpGS—p. 14 PARENT MANAGEMENT TRAINING (PMT) FOR CHILDREN WITH DISRUPTIVE BEHAVIOR DISORDERS: MOTIVATIONAL ASPECTS BY Anne Marie Caruso A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY DEPARTMENT OF COUNSELING, EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY, AND SPECIAL EDUCATION 2000 ABSTRACT PARENT MANAGEMENT TRAINING (PMT) FOR CHILDREN WITH DISRUPTIVE BEHAVIOR DISORDERS: MOTIVATIONAL ASPECTS Current well-developed lines of theory and research on parent management training for children with disruptive behavior disorders focus on predictors of outcomes, especially as they apply to demographic and environmental variables. In particular, a great deal is known about the following traditionally examined predictors: single parenthood, low socio-economic status, maternal depression, number of psychosocial stressors, and severity of the child’s behavior problem. Much less is known about potential motivational predictors in parent management training. The current study investigated parent’s motivational aspects and resources that might be included in a central schema in conjunction with traditionally examined factors which predict outcomes in parent management training. Featured motivational aspects included parent self efficacy, attributions, achievement strivings, goal theory, implicit theories, and self schemas. These motivational resources were examined within the context of vaIue/interest/appreciation aspects and integrated into a cohesive model of motivational schema through factor analysis of the Parent Motivational Schema Scale. Outcomes of parent management training were compared across various levels of traditional predictors, motivational schemata, and individual motivational resources in a canonical correlation, discriminant functional analysis, logistic regression, and series of chi-square analyses. The results indicate that motivational measures can make significant contributions and have future implications for parent management training particularly in decisions regarding customized additional components in treatment delivery for a particular parent (or subgroups of parents). Copyright by ANNE MARIE CARUSO 2000 To my parents (Matilde and Ernesto) and all other parents ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS I would like to thank Dr. Harvey Clarizio, dissertation chair, for his guidance, direction, and support which provided the structure and organization for this project. The dissertation committee in a collaborative effort, made invaluable contributions in their areas of expertise. The following were most salient: Dr. Lois Bader for her guidance in literacy analysis and aspects of the scale; Dr. Walter Hapkiewicz for his direction in instructional approaches for treatment delivery; and Dr. Michael Reckcase for his statistical expertise. I would like to thank all of the other social scientists who have contributed in large and small fashion. Dr. Jere Brophy was largely instrumental in the development and especially the content validity analysis of the PMSS. Dr. Jayne Eyberg graciously sent a copy of her Therapy Attitude Inventory and granted permission for its use in the current study. The professors, both within Michigan State and in other institutions have taught me that discovering new knowledge and disseminating information “is what academia is all about”. I would like to thank the management team and the board of directors at the Group Health Centre organization who, collectively, provided the approval and the practical means with which to carry out this project. Special thanks should go to Robert Backstrom, Chief Executive Officer, who expressed interest and kept abreast of the progress of this study. The physicians at the Group Health Centre (particularly, Dr. Thomas Muhlstein and Dr. David Burrows, Paediatricians), the psychologists and social workers at Algoma Child and Youth Services, ADHD Clinic (particularly Dr. Lina Girard, Dr. Keith Lefave, and Leanne Murray), and the many teachers from the Sault Ste Marie School Boards who referred various clients and espoused the value of this project. This project truly felt like a collaborative effort among agencies historically known for their territoriality. I would like to thank the university undergraduate and graduate students who helped manage the data. Michael lanni collected and input the data for the pilot survey. Paul DeMarco input the data for the larger, main study. I would like to thank my weekly squash partners (Eva Lidstone, Peter Schwartz, and Scott McGiIIvray) who not only help keep me sane but whose pursuit of excellence motivates me to do better. I would like to thank “the girls" (Anna Magli-Smith, Jo Thorman, Nancy Barbon, Donna Magli and Nancy Vardy) who are lifelong friends spread throughout the globe but still close. I would like to thank my family. Matilde Caruso (mother) and Santa DeMarco (sister) gave me ongoing unconditional support in so many ways. Stan Caruso (brother), took on the paternal father figure role early and continues to give vi immeasurable advice. Patricia Caruso (sister-in-law) acted as consultant and advisor through many “adverse” situations. Orlando DeMarco (brother-in-law), was always there to help out. My nephews and nieces (Paul and Patricia DeMarco and Timothy and Sarah Caruso) who continually tell me how proud they are of me and that I am their role model. It is their goals, dreams, and accomplishments of which I am so proud. I thank this family whose love, caring, entrepreneurial spirit and fortitude, motivate me to be better. vii Table of Contents List of Tables .................................................................................... xi List of Figures ................................................................................... xv List of Abbreviations ........................................................................... xvi Chapter 1 .............................................................................................................. 1 INTRODUCTION ............................................................................................... 1 Statement of the Problem .............................................................................. 1 Definitions ...................................................................................................... 4 Children with Disruptive Behavior Disorders .............................................. 4 Parent Management Training ................................................................... 13 Questions Leading to Hypotheses ............................................................... 16 Organization of the Dissertation ................................................................... 17 Chapter 2 ............................................................................................................ 19 LITERATURE REVIEW ................................................................................... 19 Universality Of Parent Management Training .............................................. 19 PMT as Prevention For Behavior Problems ............................................. 19 PMT As A Cross Categorical Treatment For All Dismptive Behaviors ..... 25 Predictors In Parent Training Interventions .................................................. 41 Traditionally Examined Outcomes And Predictors ................................... 41 Single Parent Families (SPF) ............................................................... 47 Socioeconomic Status (SES) ................................................................ 48 Parent Psychopathology ....................................................................... 49 Psychosocial Stressors ......................................................................... 50 Severity of Child Behavior .................................................................... 51 Other Predictors ................................................................................... 52 Failure to Find Consistent Predictors ....................................................... 52 Parent Cognitions As Predictors .................................................................. 59 Interactions Among Teps And Parent Cognitions ........................................ 63 Potential Motivational Predictors Of Outcomes In Pmt ................................ 65 Self Efficacy Theory ................................................................................. 70 Personal Control Beliefs ........................................................................... 78 Achievement Strivings .............................................................................. 82 Goal Theories ....................................................................................... 84 Implicit Theory .......................................................................................... 90 The Self .................................................................................................... 94 Integrating Inner Motivational Resources ................................................. 96 Pilot Study ..................................................................................................... 106 The Development Of The Parent Motivation Schema Scale ......................... 106 Method ....................................................................................................... 106 viii Participants ............................................................................................ 1 06 Measure ................................................................................................. 107 Response Scale ..................................................................................... 110 Contributions Towards Validity And Reliability ....................................... 111 Phase 1: Establishing Item Content .................................................... 112 Phase 2: Administering The Scale .................................................... 113 Phase 3: Evaluating The Factor Structure Of The Scale .................... 113 Phase 4: Evaluating Reliability Of The Scale ...................................... 114 Results ....................................................................................................... 115 Discussion ................................................................................................. 127 Hypotheses ................................................................................................ 1 30 Chapter 3 .......................................................................................................... 133 METHODOLOGY .............................................................................................. 133 Participants ................................................................................................ 1 33 Procedures ................................................................................................ 1 34 Setting and Materials ................................................................................. 137 Data Collection .......................................................................................... 139 Traditionally Examined Predictors: ......................................................... 141 Parent Motivational Aspects/ Schema .................................................... 143 Outcomes ............................................................................................... 146 Child Behavior Changes ..................................................................... 147 Parent Satisfaction ............................................................................. 147 Parent Engagement ............................................................................ 148 Design and Analysis .................................................................................. 150 Analysis I: Treatment Effects .................................................................. 150 Analysis ll: Linear Relations Among Continuous Variables .................... 152 Analysis lll: Classifying According to Drop Out ...................................... 153 Analysis lV: Using Motivational Subscales Classify Drop Out ................ 154 Analysis V: Univariate Examination of Discrete Variables ...................... 154 Analysis Vl: Change in Parents’ Motivational Schema ........................... 155 Chapter 4 .......................................................................................................... 156 RESULTS ...................................................................................................... 156 Analysis I: Treatment Effects ..................................................................... 156 Analysis II: Linear Relations Among Continuous Variables ....................... 168 Analysis lll: Classifying According to Drop Out.............. ............................ 178 Analysis IV: Individual Motivational Resources .......................................... 192 Analysis V: Univariate Examination of Discrete Variables ......................... 196 Analysis Vl: Changes in Parent Motivational Schema ............................... 199 Chapter 5 .......................................................................................................... 202 DISCUSSION ................................................................................................ 202 Summary of Results .................................................................................. 202 Hypothesis I: Treatment Effects ............................................................. 202 Hypotheses II, III, and N (All Predictors and Outcomes) ....................... 207 ix Hypothesis V (Individual Predictors) ...................................................... 210 Hypothesis VI (Changes in Motivational Aspects due to Participation) ..211 Further Comments ..................................................................................... 212 Limitations of the Present Study ................................................................ 215 Instrumentation ...................................................................................... 21 5 Sampling Issues ..................................................................................... 219 Random Assignment .............................................................................. 221 Developmental Considerations .............................................................. 222 Subgroups and/or Comorbidity Considerations ...................................... 223 Treatment Integrity ................................................................................. 223 Reactivity ................................................................................................ 224 Chapter 6 .......................................................................................................... 226 SUMMARY AND IMPLICATIONS ................................................................. 226 Implications for Theory .............................................................................. 226 Implications for Practice ............................................................................. 227 Parent Efficacy and Outcomes in Practice ............................................. 228 Parent Personal Control Beliefs and Practice ........................................ 230 Learned Helplessness in Practice .......................................................... 232 Goal Theory and PMT Practice .............................................................. 232 Self Schemas, Possible Selves, and Practice ........................................ 233 Value Aspects and Practice ................................................................... 234 Summary ................................................................................................... 235 Implications For Future Research .............................................................. 237 Final Thoughts ........................................................................................... 240 APPENDIX A .................................................................................................... 241 Parent Motivational Schema Scale (PMSS) .................................................. 241 APPENDIX B .................................................................................................... 245 Consent Forms .............................................................................................. 245 APPENDIX C .................................................................................................... 252 Decisions on Factor Dimensions ................................................................... 252 APPENDIX D .................................................................................................... 257 Logistic Regression Results Using Factor Scales in Lieu of Subscales ........ 257 APPENDIX E .................................................................................................... 261 Method Of Calculating Socio-Economic Status ............................................. 261 APPENDIX F .................................................................................................... 263 Power Analysis Results for T-Tests ............................................................... 263 BIBLIOGRAPHY ............................................................................................... 264 Table 1 Table 2 Table 3 Table 4 Table 5 Table 6 Table 7 Table 8 Table 9 Table 10 Table 11 Table 12 Table 13 Table 14 Table 15 Table 16 Table 17 Table 18 LIST OF TABLES The Individuals with Disabilities Act (IDEA) Definition Used to Describe Children with Behavior Disorders ...................................... 5 The Council for Exceptional Children Definition Used to Describe Children with Behavior Disorders ....................................... 7 Diagnostic Criteria for ADHD ............................................................ 8 Diagnostic Criteria for ODD ............................................................... 9 Diagnostic Criteria for CD ............................................................... 10 Select List of Well Known PMT Programs ............................................ 15 Summaries of DSM Nomenclatures and Prevailing Theoretical Views of ADHD ............................................................... 28 A Comparison of Disruptive Behavior Disorders .................................... 42 Summary of Studies Examining Predictors in Parent Training ................. 52 Studies Examining Motivational Variables within PMT or Similar Intervention Contexts ............................................................. 67 Explanatory Style Categorized Within the Three Attributional Dimensions Regarding Child Misbehavior .......................................... 79 Hypothesized Parent Strategies for Responding to PMT as Related to their Expectancy x Value Perceptions ................................. 97 Demographic Characteristics of Parent Participants in PMSS Pilot Study (n = 200) and Treatment and Control Parents (n=93) ............ 108 Demographic Characteristics of Children Whose Parents Completed the Pilot PMSS (n=293) ................................................... 109 Correlations among the motivation variables, statistical means, standard deviations, and reliability analysis of motivational aspects of the PMSS ................................................................................. 116 Eigenvalues, Percentages of Variance, and Cumulative Percentages for Factors of the 44 - Item Parent Motivation Schema Scale (PMSS) ...... 118 Summary of Items and Factor Loadings of Varimax Orthogonal Five-Factor Solution ......................................................... 119 Factor Loadings for Varimax Orthoganal F ive-Factor Solution .................. 121 xi Table 19 Table 20 Table 21 Table 22 Table 23 Table 24 Table 25 Table 26 Table 27 Table 28 Table 29 Table 30 Table 31 Table 32 Table 33 Table 34 LIST OF TABLES Descriptives for Varimax Orthoganal Five-Factor Solution on all 144 items of the PMSS (n=293) ......................................................... 123 Correlations among Motivation Measures pre- PMT and post- PMT Program for Treatment (n=64 ) and Control (n=29) ........................ 124 Correlations among Motivation Measures pre- PMT and post- PMT Program for all Participants (n=93) ............................................. 125 Correlations among Motivation Schema pre- PMT and post- PMT Programfor Treatment (n=64 ) and Control (n=29) ........................ 126 Test-Retest Reliability Coefficients pre- PMT and post- PMT Program for Treatment (n=64 ) and Control (n=29) ........................ 128 Demographic Characteristics of Participants in PMT Program for Treatment (n=64 ) and Control (n=29) ........................ 135 Demographic Characteristics of Children Whose Parents Participated in the PMT Study (n=93) ................................................ 136 Step by Step Procedures as Detailed in Defiant Children (Barkley, 1997) .............................................................................. 140 Factor Loadings on the Therapy Attitude Inventory (TAI) ........................ 149 Summary of Specific Hypotheses (abbreviated), Measures, and Analyses .................................................................. 151 Mean Participant Scores (Standard Deviations) on Continuous Outcome Variables ......................................................... 157 Mean Change Scores (Standard Deviations) from CBCL Measures ........................................................................... 164 Analysis of Variance Results for Main Effects and Interaction Effects of Group, Time Point, and Scale on Child Behavior Changes (CBCL) ...................................................... 165 Correlations and Standardized Canonical Coefficients and Their Canonical Variates in Separate Models ................................. 169 Correlations and Standardized Canonical Coefficients and Their Canonical Variates in a Combined Model .............................. 171 Canonical Analysis of Predictors and Outcome Variables in PMT ........................................................................... 172 xii Table 35 Table 36 Table 37 Table 38 Table 39 Table 40 Table 41 Table 42 Table 43 Table 44 Table 45 Table 46 Table 47 Table 48 Table 49 LIST OF TABLES Correlations and Standardized Canonical Coefficients Between Traditional and Motivational Predictors and PMT Outcome Variables and Their Canonical Variates in a Combined Model without SES ................................................... 174 Correlations, Standardized Canonical Coefficients and Their Canonical Variates inTraditional and Motivational Models ....................................................................................... 175 Correlations, Standardized Canonical Coefficients and Their Canonical Variates in a Combined Model .............................. 176 Canonical Analysis of Predictors and Outcome Variables in PMT ........................................................................... 177 Means and Standard Deviations of Predictor Variables as a Function of PMT Drop Out Point ................................................ 179 Predictor Variables in Discriminant Function Analysis by Traditional, Motivational, and Combined Models .............................. 181 Correlation of Predictor Variables with Discriminant Functions (Function Structure Matrix) and Standardized Discriminant Function Coefficients ..................................................................... 182 Correlations Between Discriminating Variables and Discriminant Functions (Function Structure Matrix) ................................................ 183 Classification Analysis for Drop Out Using Traditional Predictors Only ............................................................................................. 184 Classification Analysis for Drop Out Using Motivational Predictors Only ............................................................................................. 188 Classification Analysis for Drop Out Using Both Traditional and Motivational Predictors in a Combined Model ................................. 191 Logistic Regression Predicting PMT Drop Out .................................... 195 Prevalence (%) of Statistics Canada Income Cutoffs within Points of Drop Out ........................................................................ 197 Prevalence (%) of Early Developmental Milestones within Points of Drop Out ........................................................................ 197 Chi-Square Analyses of Participant Characteristics within Points of Drop Out ........................................................................ 198 xiii Table 50 Table 51 Table 52 Table 53 Table A1 Table A2 Table 0.1 Table C.2 Table C.3 Table 0.4 Table D1 Table 0.2 Table E.1 Table E.2 Table F.1 LIST OF TABLES Mean Change Scores and T-test for Pre- and Post- PMSS Subscales by Treatment (n=37) and Control Group (n=29) ................................................................................. 200 Mean Change Scores and T-test for Pre- and Post- PMSS Factors by Treatment (n=37) and Control Group (n=29) ................................................................................. 201 Summary of Specific Hypotheses (abbreviated), Analyses, and Decisions with Predictors ............................................................ 203 Salient Motivational Aspects and Value Interventions as Potential SMEPT modules ............................................................... 236 Agreement Among Independent Raters ............................................. 242 PMSS Items Listed by Motivational Resource ....................................... 245 Descriptives for Varimax Orthoganal Nine-Factor Solution ..................... 256 Eigenvalues, Percentages of Variance, and Cummulative Percentages for Nine Factor Solution ................................................ 257 Summary of Item and Factor Loadings for Nine-Factor Solution ............. 258 Descriptives for Non-orthogonal Five Factor Solution ........................... 259 Logistic Regression Predicting PMT Drop Out Using Factor Scores ......... 261 Correlation of Individual Items of the PMSS with Drop Out Outcome ......... 262 Hollingshead Two Factor Index ......................................................... 264 Low Income Cutoffs ........................................................................ 265 Power Analysis Results .................................................................. 266 xiv Figure 1 Figure 2 Figure 3 Figure 4 Figure 5 Figure 6 Figure 7 Figure 8 Figure 9 Figure 10 Figure 11 LIST OF FIGURES Scree Plot Yielding Five Factor Solution ............................................... 117 Pre- Post- Mean Scores of CBCL for Treatment and Control Groups ......... 158 Median CBCL Results Across All Scales .............................................. 159 Median CBCL Total Subscale Results ................................................ 160 Median CBCL Internalizing Subscale Results.........................................161 Median CBCL Externalizing Subscale Results ....................................... 162 Box Plot Depicting Interaction of Group * Scale Results ........................... 166 Bar Graph Depicting Interaction of Group * Scale Results ........................ 167 Territorial Map with Traditional Model .................................................. 186 Territorial Map with Motivational Model ................................................ 189 Territorial Map with Combined Model ................................................... 193 XV LIST OF ABBREVIATIONS SMePT ....................................................... Schema Mediated Parent Training PMT .......................................................... Parent Management Training ADHD ........................................................ Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder ODD .......................................................... Oppositional Defiant Disorder CD ............................................................ Conduct Disorder DSM-IV ...................................................... Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders - Fourth Edition PMSS ........................................................ Parent Motivation Schema Scale TEP ........................................................... Traditionally Examined Predictors SE ............................................................ Self Efficacy OES .......................................................... Optimistic Explanatory Style PES ........................................................... Pessimistic Explanatory Style Sch ........................................................... Schematicity Imp ............................................................ Implicit Theory PGD .......................................................... Performance Goals-Self Defeating PGE .......................................................... Performance Goals-Self Enhancing LG ............................................................ Learning Goals Val ............................................................ Value CBCL ......................................................... Child Behavior Checklist (4-18) HSQ .......................................................... Home Situations Questionnaire TAI ............................................................ Therapy Attitude Inventory BDI-ll ......................................................... Beck Depression Inventory- 2nd Ed. LICO ......................................................... Low Income Cutoffs SPF ........................................................... Single Parent Family LV ............................................................. Eactor1 (LG, Val) IMC ........................................................... Factor 2 (SE, OES. Sch) PGE .......................................................... Factor 3 (PGE, 1 PGD) PGD .......................................................... Factor 4 (PGD) IT .............................................................. Factor 5 (Imp) xvi Chapter 1 INTRODUCTION Statement of the Problem Parent behavior management training (PMT) is a widely studied intervention for children with disruptive behavior disorders. Outcome studies have demonstrated considerable success in reducing symptoms in children. As well, studies suggest that parent management training (PMT) can also be viewed as widely applicable. First, parent management training is an effective prevention method for the development of disruptive behaviors (Adelman & Taylor, 1997; Hinshaw, Klein, & Abikoff, 1998). Second, put within the context of the shifting conceptionalizations of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) and the development of comorbid disorders including Oppositional Defiant Disorder (ODD), Conduct Disorder (CD), and possibly other disorders, it can be argued that selection of specific PMT strategies should not hinge on a specific diagnosis but on other factors. Furthermore, disruptive behaviors are found among children with various diagnoses (including developmental disorders) and children who do not meet criteria for any particular diagnosis (Johnston, 1996). Finally, the majority of referrals for mental services are due to disruptive behaviors, regardless of diagnostic status (Kazdin, 1987). PMT therapies which are concerned with specific childhood outcomes need not be isolated to specific childhood categorical labels but include all disruptive behavior problems and address several additional (and possibly) interacting predictors. Previous studies in parent training have recurrently 1 examined several predictors of successful outcomes in PMT, usually measured as child behavior changes. Among the predictors examined are the following: marital status (Holden, 1990; Webster-Stratton, 1985; Webster-Stratton & Hammond, 1990), socio-economic status (Holden, 1990; Prinz & Miller, 1994; Webster-Stratton, 1985; Webster-Stratton & Hammond, 1990), parent psychopathology and/or depression (Frankel & Simmons, 1992; Webster- Stratton, 1985; Webster-Stration & Hammond, 1990), severity of the child’s problem (Ruma, 1996), and number of life stressors (Prinz & Miller, 1994; Webster-Stratton & Hebert, 1993). These predictors are largely demographic and environmental. Little research has been devoted to examining other predictors and little research has explicitly addressed potential mediators in parent training outcomes. This is where the challenge reportedly lies in research (Kazdin & Weisz, 1998). For behavioral outcomes, it seems reasonable to examine cognitions related to the individual-environment interaction. Parent cognitions may be organized in “schemas” which encompass a wide range of constructs. The notion that parental beliefs and attitudes should be considered in selecting strategies to teach effective parenting can be supported by bodies of research literature specific to social science research in parenting and to educational research in motivation. First, research studies have been conducted to demonstrate relationships between parent beliefs and attitudes and children’s behavior (Goodnow, 1995; Holden, 1995; Lisi & Sigel, 1995). Surprisingly, parent management training research which has examined specific predictors and childhood outcomes, has, for the most part, ignored these potential cognitive mediational predictors, until recently (Johnston, 1996). Second, considerable research has examined the relationships among motivational concepts and Ieaming outcomes (Brophy, 1999; Reeve, 1996). Since PMT involves educational and learning components, considerable potential exists for parent motivational aspects acting as mediators in parent training outcomes. Within this second body of research, less emphasis has been given to value aspects of motivation which imply that some relevance to personal schemas must be perceived in order for Ieamers to benefit from the activity (Brophy, 1998). This value aspect certainly would contribute significantly to parent expectations regarding PMT and ultimately, outcomes. The purpose of the present proposal was to examine parent’s motivational resources, as individual constructs and as integrated motivation schemas as mediators of predictors of outcomes in parent training. It was hypothesized that these motivational aspects are mediators of traditionally examined predictors and have implications for the predictive success of parent training outcomes. Specifically, the present study investigated the relations between parent’s inner motivational resources (self efficacy, attribution, goal, implicit, and self schema theories) and value as related to motivation, and how these predict PMT outcomes as measured by child behavior changes, parent satisfaction, and parent involvement. It was expected that these findings would culminate in a solid rationale and empirical basis for the development and inclusion of Schema Mediated Parent Training (SMePT) components/modules into traditional PMTs. Definitions To better understand the contents of this dissertation, two central and broad categories of terms must be defined. These include definitions for behavior disorders as well as definitions for parent management training. Children with Disruptive _Behavior Disorders Many approaches have been used to classify children with behavior disorders. Notably, the term, behavior disorders has been used to describe children with both external (social, directed at others) and internal (emotional, directed at self) problems (Hardman, Drew, & Egan, 1999). Children with externalizing or disruptive behaviors gather the most attention from parents and teachers and are the most commonly referred (Kazdin & Weisz, 1998). Children with internalizing disorders (eg. depression, anxiety, and withdrawal) are more likely to be overlooked (Kazdin & Weisz, 1998); yet, it is this latter group which qualifies for special education services under the Individuals with Disabilities Education Act (IDEA) (1997). The IDEA describes children with behavior disorders under its Seriously Emotionally Disturbed (SED) definition (Tablet). The current definition states that SED is “a condition resulting in persistent and consistent maladaptive behavior which interferes with the student’s Ieaming process” (Hard man, 1999, p. 219). This federal definition excludes children with social maladjustment (such as Table 1 The lndivfiuals with Disabilities Act (IDEA) definition y§ed to describe children with behavior disorders. “Seriously emotionally disturbed” is defined as . .: 1. a condition exhibiting one or more of the following characteristics over a long period of time and to a marked degree, which adversely affects educational performance: a. b. c. d. e. an inability to learn which cannot be explained by intellectual, sensory or health factors; an inability to build or maintain satisfactory relationships with peers and teachers; inappropriate types of behavior or feelings under normal circumstances; a general pervasive mood of unhappiness or depression; or a tendency to develop physical symptoms or fears associated with personal or school problems 2. the term includes children who are schizophrenic or autistic. 3. the term does not include children who are socially maladjusted, unless it is determined that they are seriously disturbed. (adapted from Hardman, 1999 , p. 219) Conduct Disorders below) from satisfying criteria for a Serious Emotional Disturbance (SED) (Forness et al., 1996). However, children with externalizing problems can also qualify under the IDEA. For example, children with ADHD can qualify under PoHI and can also qualify under Section 504 of the Rehabilitation Act of 1973 which can also lead to special education or accommodations in general education. The Council for Exceptional Children (CEC) has proposed a more inclusive definition which has significant advantages over the IDEA definition (Table 2). These advantages include a) recognizing impairments in adaptive social, emotional, and behavioral areas; b) using multiple sources, considering cultural and ethnic factors, and using normative standards; c) examining prereferral and other interventions before labeling; and, d) potentially including children labeled as socially maladjusted (Hardman et al., 1999). The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (4Th ed.; DSM- IV; American Psychiatric Association, 1994), classifies disruptive behavior disorders as Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (Table 3), Oppositional Defiant Disorder (Table 4), and Conduct Disorders (Table 5). Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorders are characterized by the presence of six or more criteria within two broad categories: a) inattention and b) hyperactivity/impuIsivity (American Psychiatric Association, 1994). Oppositional Defiant Disorders are characterized by the presence of at least four behaviors consistent with a pattern of negativistic, defiant, disobedient, and hostile behavior towards authority figures (APA, 1994). Conduct Disorders (CD) are characterized by the presence of Table 2 The Council for Exceptional Children definition used to describe children with behavior disorders. Emotional or behavior disorder (EBD) refers to a condition in which behavioral or emotional responses of an individual in school are so different from his/her generally accepted age-appropriate, ethnic, or cultural norms that they adversely affect educational performance in such areas as self-care, social relationships, personal adjustment, academic progress, classroom behavior, or work adjustment. EBD is more than a transient, expected response to stressors in the child’s or youth’s environment and would persist even with individualized interventions, such as feedback to the individual, consultation with parents or families, and/or modification of the educational environment. The eligibility decision must be based on multiple sources of data about the individual’s behvioral or emotional functioning. EBD must be exhibited in at least two different settings at least one of which must be school related. EBD can coexist with other handicapping conditions as defined elsewhere in this law [IDEA]. This category may include children or youth with schizophrenia, affective disorders, or with other sustained disturbances of conduct, attention, or adjustment (Adapted from Hardman, 1999, p. 220) Table 3 Diagnostic criteria for Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Dmrder (ADHD). A. Either (1) or (2): (1) six (or more) of the following symptoms of inattention have persisted for at least 6 months to a degree that is maladaptive and inconsistent with developmental level. Inattention (a) often fails to give close attention to details or makes careless mistakes in schoolwork, work, or other activities (b) often has difficulty sustaining attention in tasks or play activities (c) often does not seem to listen when spoken to directly (d) often does not follow through on instructions and fails to finish schoolwork, chores, or duties in the workplace (not due to oppositional behavior or failure to understand instructions) (e) often has difficulty organizing tasks and activities (f) often avoids, dislikes, or is reluctant to engage in tasks that require sustained mental effort (such as schoolwork or homework) (9) often loses things necessary for tasks or activities (e.g. toys, school assignments, pencils, books, or tools) (h) is often easily distracted by extraneous stimuli (i) is often forgetful in daily activities (2) six (or more) of the following symptoms of hyperactivity-impulsivity have persisted for at least 6 months to a degree that is maladaptive and inconsistent with developmental level: Hyperactivity (a) often fidgets with hands or feet or squirms in seat (b) often leaves seat in classroom or in other situations in which remaining seated is expected (c) often runs about or climbs excessively in situations in which it is inappropriate (in adolescents or adults, may be limited to subjective feelings of restlessness) (d) often has difficulty playing or engaging in leisure activities quietly (e) is often “on the go” or often acts as if “driven by a motor" (f) often talks excessively Impulsivity (9) often blurts out answers before questions have been completed (h) often has difficulty awaiting turn (i) often interrupts or intrudes on others (e.g. butts into conversations or games) Onset before age 7 Impairment present in two or more settings Clear evidence of impairment in social, academic, or occupational functioning Symptoms do not occur exclusively during the course of a Pervasive Developmental Disorder, Schizophrenia, or other Psychotic Disorder and are not better accounted for by another mental disorder (e.g. Mood Disorder, Anxiety Disorder, Dissociative Disorder, or a Personality Disorder) Code based on type: AttentionDeficit/Hyperactivity Disorder, Combined Type Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder, Predominantly lnattentive Type Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder, Predominantly Hyperactive-Impulsive Type [119,093 (American Psychiatric Association, 1994, pp. 84-85). Table 4 Djaqnostic criteriafir Oppositional Defi_ant Disorder (ODD). A. A pattern of negativistic, hostile, and defiant behavior lasting at least 6 months, during which four (or more) of the following are present: (1) often loses temper (2) often argues with adults (3) often actively defies or refuses to comply with “adults” requests or rules (4) often deliberately annoys people (5) often blames others for his or her mistakes or misbehavior (6) is often touchy or easily annoyed by others (7) is often angry or resentful (8) is often spiteful or vindictive Note: Consider a criteria met only if the behavior occurs more frequently than is typically observed in individuals of comparable age and developmental level. B. The disturbance in behavior causes clinically significant impairment in social, academic, or occupational functioning. The behaviors do not occur exclusively during the course of a Psychotic or Mood Disorder. . Criteria are not met for Conduct Disorder, and, if the individual is age 18 years or older, criteria are not met for Antisocial Personality Disorder. (APA, 1994, pp. 93-94). Table 5 Criterig for Conduct [lsorder (CDL A. A repetitive and persistent pattern of behavior in which the basic rights others or major age-appropriate societal norms or rules are violated, as manifested by the presence of three (or more) of the following criteria in the past 12 months, with at least one criteria present in the past 6 months: Aggression to people and animals (1) often bullies, threatens, or intimidates others (2) often initiates fights (3) has used a weapon that can cause serious physical harm to others (e.g. a bat, brick, broken bottle, knife, gun) (4) has been physically cruel to people (5) has been physically cruel to animals (6) has stolen while confronting a victim (e.g., mugging, purse, snatching, extortion, armed robbery) (7) has forced someone into sexual activity Destruction of property (8) has deliberately engaged in fire setting with the intention of causing serious damage (9) has deliberately destroyed others' property (other than by fire setting) Deceitfulness or theft (10) has broken into someone else’s house, building, or car (11) often lies to obtain goods or favors or to avoid obligations (i.e., “cons” others) (12) has stolen items of nontrivial value without confronting a victim (e.g. shoplifting, but without breaking and entering; forgery) Serious violation of rules (13) often stays out at night despite parental prohibitions, beginning before age 13 years (14) has run away from home overnight at least twice while living in parental or parental surrogate home (or once without returning for a lengthy period) (15) is often truant from school, beginning before age 13 years 8. The disturbance in behavior causes clinically significant impairment in social, academic, or occupational functioning. C. If the individual is age 18 or older, criteria are not met for Antisocial Personality Disorder. Specify type based on age at onset: Childhood-Onset Type: onset of at least criterion characteristic prior to age 10 Adolescent-Onset Type: absence of any criterion characteristic prior to age 10 Specify severity: Mild: few if any conduct problems in excess of those required for diagnosis and conduct problems cause only minor harm to others Moderate: number of conduct problems and effect on others intermediate between “mild” and “severe” Severe: many conduct problems in excess of those required to make the diagnosis or conduct problems cause considerable harm to others (APA, 1994. pp. 90-91) 10 three or more criteria within four broad categories: a) aggression to people and animals, b) destruction of property, 0) deceitfulness or theft, and d) serious violations of rules (APA, 1994) (Table 5). A central feature of conduct disorder is a persistent pattern of behavior in which the rights of others and age-appropriate social norms are violated (Kazdin, 1997, p. 162). In the research literature, several terms are sometimes used interchangeably with conduct disorders, all having to do with varying degrees of conduct related problems. Juvenile delinquency, (a less inclusive, legal term) and antisocial behavior are among those terms most closely associated with Conduct Disorders. Violence is defined as aggressive acts causing serious harm, such as aggravated assault, rape, robbery, and homicide (Loeber & Stouthamer-Loeber, 1998, p.242). Aggression refers to acts that inflict bodily harm or mental harm on others and is confined to acts that cause less serious harm or mental harm on others (Loeber & Stouthamer-Loeber, 1998, p. 242). Thus, aggressive acts are viewed as less serious than violence which is an extreme form of antisocial or delinquent behavior (T olan & Guerra, 1994, p.3). In examining the criteria for conduct disorders, some children with conduct disorders may exhibit behaviors consistent with antisocial, delinquent, violent, and/or aggressive acts and some may not, depending upon the constellation of symptoms. For the purposes of this paper, the distinctions among the disruptive behavior disorders and these other acts/areas will be retained when required; however, results from studies examining each of these concepts will be discussed. 11 While recognized in the DSM-IV (APA, 1994), Conduct Disorders (per se) do not meet the federal guidelines under the IDEA as a disability condition (Murray & Myers, 1998). In fact, little concordance has been reported between DSM-IV diagnosis and eligibility for special education services under the category of SED under the IDEA (Fomess et al., 1996). This may lead to some compensatory strategies on the part of educational personnel which can be deemed inappropriate and unethical. For example, school personnel may misclassify children with CD as SED as the “path of least resistance” and misplace them in special education self-contained classes for SED (Murray & Myers, 1998). Children with CD tend not to do well in special education settings and tend to be disruptive with children who are genuinely emotionally disturbed (Murray 8. Myers, 1998). Furthermore, putting both disorders within the same educational setting may exacerbate the problems since educational programs and treatments for children with conduct disorders and serious emotional disturbances are not the same (Murray & Myers, 1998). Clarizio (1996) further differentiates this distinction by suggesting that placement in special education programs and related interventions for SED is contraindicated for the children in the late onset CD group. Another concern has been voiced that children who are socially maladjusted have learned their behavior patterns and should not be served under the SED definition which has been interpreted to address only those behavioral disorders that have an emotional causation (McIntyre & Forness, 1996). The definition of SED continues to need further revisions (Murray & Myers, 1998) despite various previous 12 proposals and attempts towards potential changes (McIntyre, 1993; McIntyre & Fomess, 1996; Sweeney, 1995). Since the IDEA definition for behavior disorders continues to be criticized on these same grounds, i.e., for excluding children described as socially maladjusted (Hardman et al., 1999), the focus of the current paper will be on disruptive behavior disorders as defined by the DSM-IV. Parent Management Trainina Major treatments for children with behavior disorders address a variety of general goals within a variety of approaches including the following: a) insight oriented therapy (for relieving symptoms, treating causes of behavior, developing therapeutic relationships, understanding unconscious causes of behavior; b) play therapy (for developing therapeutic relationships, playing out emotional problems, developing positive peer relationships, teaching social skills, developing problem solving skills; 0) behavior therapy (for developing positive peer relationships, teaching language, self-help, academic, social skills and controlling disordered, unusual, aggressive and general overt behavior); d) marital and family therapy (for treating causes of behavior, developing therapeutic relationships, and developing problem solving skills; e) drug therapy (for relieving symptoms controlling behaviors); and f) group psychotherapy (for treating causes of behavior, developing therapeutic relationships, developing positive peer relationships, developing problem solving skills, and understanding unconscious causes of behavior (Hardman et al., 1999). As noted above, the selection of the most efficacious approach may be dependent, in part, upon the classification of behavior disorder. 13 Kazdin (1997) identified four psychosocial treatments which are reported to have considerable promise in treating conduct disorders. Using carefully selected criteria for evaluation, Parent Management Training (PMT) was identified as among the most well developed and researched. PMT primarily consists of training parents to alter their child’s behavior in the home (and other settings) through changing coercive, unsupportive patterns of interchanges between parent and child into more prosocial and reinforcing interchanges (Kazdin, 1997). Using a more stringent criteria of treatments, Kazdin and Weisz (1998), identified only three treatments in the efficacious category (i.e., established), again including PMT. Efficacious treatments have been shown to be more effective than no treatment, placebo, and other treatments with findings replicated by multiple and different researchers (Kazdin & Weisz, 1998). Brestan and Eyberg (1998) describe PMT as having one of the strongest bases in controlled outcome research for children with behavior disorders. Webster- Stratton (1993) describes PMT as having the most effective and promising results with children with ODD or CD. A plethora of PMT programs exist and have been shown to be efficacious in treating the disruptive behavior disorders (ADHD, ODD, and CD) as well as a wide domain of child problems and ages (Table 6). These PMT programs can be primarily classified as behavioral management programs or based on operant learning principles. The behavioral PMT programs have several elements in common as follows: a) a focus on accurate descriptions of child behavior; b) the identification of antecedents and consequences; c) an emphasis on teaching 14 Table 6 Select List of Well Known PMT Proqrams Author(s) Program Description or Model or Treatment Manual Name Approach Barkley (1997) Defiant Children behavioral Bloomquist (1996) Skills Training for Children with Behavior combination Dinkmeyer & McKay (1989) Eyberg (1998) Forgatch & Patterson (1989) Forehand & Manon (1981) Fox & Fox (1990) Getz & Gunn (1988) Gordon (1975) Patterson & Forgatch (1987) Phelan (1995) Webster-Stratton (1981 ) Webster-Stratton (1996) Wells (1993) Behavior Disorders Systematic Training for Effective Parenting (STEP) Parent-Child Interaction Therapy (Special lnroads; U. of Florida) Parents and adolescents living together Helping the Non—Compliant Child STAR Parenting Program (stop, think, ask, respond) Parent education Parent Effectiveness Training (PET) Parents and adolescents living together: The Basics 1-2-3 Magic BASIC ADVANCE treatment manual and training materials with video tapes and discussion questions Parent Training Manual for Oppositional Children behavioral and cognitive behavioral Adlerian behavioral behavioral behavioral cognitive behavior modification family systems Adlerian behavioral behavioral behavioral behavioral behavioral 15 consistency; d) use of reinforcement techniques; and, e) assigned homework tasks between sessions (Hutchings, 1996). Overall, PMT refers to a set of interventions that target parenting skills critical to improving family interactions and enhancing their children’s socializing skills (Frick, 1998). The term PMT used throughout this proposal encompasses these behaviorally oriented approaches except in instances where PMT approaches need to be clearly differentiated. Interestingly, it is reported that the parent advocacy/involvement movement in children’s mental health (including PMT) has its roots in special education (Bickman, Heflinger, Northrup, Sonnichsen, & Schilling, 1998). Specifically, the IDEA explicitly includes parent participation in special education as a due process issue (Bickman et al., 1998; Heflinger, Bickman, Northrup, & Sonnichsen 1997). Finally, the term “parents” is used throughout the proposal. It is recognized that parents may include grandparents, step-parents, and other primary caregivers or guardians. Questions Leadinq to Hypotheses Little or no attention had been focused on demonstrating (a) whether parent motivational aspects influence outcomes in PMT; (b) whether parent motivational aspects change as a result of PMT participation; (0) evaluating how to maximize parent motivational aspects, and ((1) whether maximizing parent motivational aspects will lead to better outcomes. The present study addressed the first two questions as follows: 16 '__. we " “‘0'“ 1. Do certain motivational resources, either individually or as integrated schema, increase parent involvement or engagement; and, if so, does the involvement lead to changes in the way parents benefit from PMT and consequently, lead to better outcomes for the child and parent? 2. Are the levels of various parent motivational resources stable and consistent constructs or do they change as a result of participating in parent management training sessions? The main hypothesis of the study is that traditionally examined predictors and motivational aspects are predictive of positive changes in child behavior, parent satisfaction, and parent engagement. Orqgnization of the Dissertation The first chapter introduces the study, defines the problem, and briefly describes the purpose for the study. The concept of Schema Mediated Parent Training (SMePT) is introduced and several key terms (disruptive behavior disorders and parent management training) central to the arguments of the dissertation, are defined. An argument is presented for the study of motivational resources as predictors in parent management training outcome research. Chapter 2 provides a comprehensive literature review and the main body of the dissertation. Arguments for the wide use of parent behavior management training are supported by research which 1) discusses parent training as a viable prevention method for the development of disruptive behavior disorders; 2) presents a rationale for why treatment may not necessarily need to be tailored to specific, isolated, identified disorders; and, 3) outlines treatment efficacy studies supporting parent management training for disruptive behavior disorders. A detailed description is provided on traditionally examined predictors of PMT outcomes, including marital status, socioeconomic status, parent 17 psychopathology, number of psychosocial stressors, and severity of the child’s behavior disorder. Chapter 2 also presents an argument for considering parent cognitions as predictors in PMT outcomes. Specifically, research on the influence of student inner motivational resources on academic achievement is postulated to directly translate to the influence of parent inner motivational resources on parent training “achievement”. A pilot study describes the development of an instrument, the Parent Motivational Schema Scale (PMSS), which was specifically designed to measure these inner motivational resources. Chapter 3 describes the methodology, Chapter 4 presents the results, and Chapter 5 discusses the findings of the study. This latter chapter also discusses shortcomings and limitations regarding the research study. A summary containing implications for theory, future research, and practice is presented in Chapter 6. Based on findings of significant motivational predictors, Chapter 6 presents a detailed rationale towards the development of Schema Mediated Parent Training (SMePT) and clearly articulates components which may be a necessary and beneficial addition to PMT approaches. 18 Chapter 2 LITERATURE REVIEW Universality Of Pgrent Manggement Training The majority of PMT studies tend to concentrate on primary interventions for full-blown disorders and isolate specific populations for their research. Arguments can be made that parent management training may have some efficacy with all behavior populations. In several replication studies, the Webster- Stratton PMT approach has been shown to be efficacious with several different populations as well as superior to wait-list control groups (Webster-Stratton, 1993). First, PMT is a viable prevention intervention for the development of disruptive behavior disorders. Second, treatment need not necessarily be directed towards specific homogeneous populations, especially because treatment results may not necessarily be a result of the nature of children’s disorders. PMT as Prevention For Behavior Problems Cowen and Hightower (1988) define prevention according to two broad strategies: 1) “primary prevention... seeks to forestall the development of pathology” and 2) “secondary prevention seeks to identify early signs of dysfunction and to introduce prompt correctives to short circuit negative outcomes” (Cowen & Hightower, 1988, p. 777). Within the context of juvenile delinquency, Davidson and Redner (1988) consider primary prevention as intervention before behavior is labeled while secondary or tertiary prevention as 19 intervention after formal labeling. It has been reported that schools have done some work with early secondary prevention and even less with primary prevention (Cowen & Hightower, 1988). Many prevention programs seem to target populations at risk or treat identified behavior disordered populations and sometimes neglect the direct caregivers such as parents. Even when prevention programs make efficient use of supportive personnel such as child-aides, they rarely employ the use of parents as service providers. For example, the Primary Mental Health Project uses child-aides for children in primary grades at risk for academic difficulties and uses parent aides who function similar to paraprofessionals in keeping parents informed (Cowen & Hightower, 1988). There is no mention of using parents as child-aides. Other examples of prevention programs which do not overtly mention parent involvement include programs designed for children with severe behavior problems (Dyer & Larsson, 1997; Wolery & Winterling, 1997), aggression (Matson & Duncan, 1997) and noncompliant and aggressive behavior in a case study of a child, JB, who was at risk for a behavioral disorders (Umbreit & Blair, 1996). In fact, in an analysis of experimental studies on intervention for emotional and behavior disorders from 1980 to 1993, intervention types were characterized along 6 categories: skills training, self management, antecedent based, consequence based, peer mediated, and other (Dunlap & Childs, 1996). Nowhere in the article was parent training explicitly mentioned. These studies are surprising in light of the fact that Kazdin (1987) has been quoted as writing “no other intervention for antisocial children has been investigated as thoroughly 20 as parent management training and has shown as favorable results” (Strayhorn et al., 1993). In contrast, The Fast Track Multi-Site Demonstration Project directly addresses the importance of parent involvement (Bierrnan et al., 1996). Their Conduct Problems Prevention Research Group (CPPRG) advocates the need for interventions including socializing agents (parents, peers, teachers, etc.) as a secondary necessary condition in order to affect long term developmental changes in children at risk for conduct disorder (Bierrnan et al., 1996 ). It should be noted that the CPPRG view aggression and later conduct disorders as multifaceted and multiply determined (Bierrnan et al., 1996 ). As a result, they have designed a particularly extensive and intensive preventive intervention which will likely be beyond the scope of most independent agencies unless mechanisms are in place for school linked services or integrated services such as those described by Adelman and Taylor (1997): The call is for moving from fragmentation to coordinated/integrated intervention and from narrowly focused, problem-specific, and specialist-oriented services to comprehensive general approaches. (Adelman & Taylor, 1997, p.408) Similarly, comprehensive services may be made possible through integrated services among community based mental health and social service programs in a system of care defined as: a community based approach to providing comprehensive, integrated services that are available through multiple agencies and professionals, in collaboration with families. (Eber & Nelson, 1997). 21 Several current programs are emphasizing these comprehensive systems of care wherein agencies share resources (Adelman & Taylor, 1997; Eber & Nelson, 1997; Fomess et al., 1996). In addition to researching the most comprehensive of treatments, it might be beneficial for researchers to investigate the necessary and sufficient conditions, i.e., minimal services required to affect change in outcomes of at-risk children. Other studies have addressed parent training as an important intervention for children at risk for developing conduct disorders. Kaiser and Hester (1997) listed parent training as first among three components essential for prevention of conduct disorders. However, they also note that most behavioral interventions target populations already identified as behavior problems as opposed to children at risk (Kaiser & Hester, 1997). Ramey et al. (1988) wrote about the Carolina Early Intervention Program which not only directly involved parents but also considered care giver characteristics (such as expectations, propensities, development, supports, stressors, and interactions with the child) as important factors in their conceptual model for early intervention. Forness et al. (1996) recommended parent training as an initial intervention in their multistage approach to prevention and intervention of emotional behavioral disorders. Many current programs for children with SED have been criticized for emphasizing comprehensive systems of care without considering prevention in service delivery systems (McConaughy, Kay, & Fitzgerald, 1998). Davidson and Redner (1988) developed their prevention training model for at risk juvenile delinquents around a family focus. Eber and Nelson (1997) also 22 discuss a family focus as important for comprehensive and collaborative planning and service delivery in their wraparound approach. Wraparound has been described as “a needs-driven process for creating and providing services for individual children and their families” (Eber et al., 1997). This latter approach provides support for direct caregivers (whether teachers, parents, etc.) using an Interactive team to empower members. Price et al. (1989) note that not everything called a prevention program is, in fact, a prevention program. This philosophy is consistent with previous research suggesting that there have been no known studies in the long term prevention of conduct disorder (Bierrnan et al., 1996). Long term studies are necessary to evaluate if prevention interventions with children at risk for conduct and other behavior disorders are beneficial. Studies advocate up to three to five year follow ups (Kaiser & Hester, 1997) or even treatment up until children reach secondary school age (Bierrnan et al., 1996). Some studies indicate that children at risk for behavior problems have benefited from early intervention prevention programs (Ramey et al., 1988; Strayhorn & Weidman, 1991 ). Other studies suggest that, because there is such difficulty in accurately identifying high risk groups, there is no evidence that prevention affects later juvenile delinquent behavior (Davidson & Redner, 1988). Strayhorn and Weidman (1991) demonstrated that a parent-child interaction, preventive mental health intervention had beneficial long term effects. Specifically, the results of a one year follow up indicated that improvements in classroom behavior were correlated with improvements parents had shown in training. 23 Webster-Stratton (1998) evaluated the effectiveness of a parenting theory based, multi-faceted, prevention intervention which was designed to prevent the development of ODD and CD in at risk Head Start children. Results indicated that children in the parenting program showed significantly more Improvements (fewer CD problems, less ODD, less negative affect, more positive affect) than children in the control condition who had received the regular Head Start program. Tucker, Gross, Fogg, Delaney, and Lapporte (1998) recently examined the efficacy of a behavioral parent training (BPT) intervention as a prevention strategy for young children at risk of developing CDs. One of the findings of the study indicated that the amount of parent participation in the treatment was correlated with greater gains in parent-child outcomes at one year. Similarly, in a review of the literature on early intervention and prevention programs, Tucker and Gross (1997) found that PMT significantly reduced negative child behaviors, promoted positive parent child interactions, and that these treatment gains were maintained over time. The phrase has been coined that “parent training is prevention”. Furthermore, prevention activity creates a safe and productive environment and develops the type of attitudes and capacities that students and their families need to deal with violence and other threats to safety. (Adelman & Taylor, 1997, p. 417). Finally, PMT programs which view parents as equal partners in developing and implementing interventions rather than helpers while mental health professionals 24 remain the “experts”, lead to more successful outcomes (McConaughy et al., 1998). PMT As A Cross Categmn'cal Treatment For All Disrgptive Behaviors Discussion in this chapter focuses on categorical versus cross-categorical approaches, and the state of the art with DSM-IV classification systems. Both issues are used as arguments against the practice of rigidly defining target populations (based on child diagnoses) for intervention research. Cross- categorical approaches have several advantages over categorical approaches In designing intervention studies. Also, DSM-IV classifications are far from reliable and consistent. In addition, comorbidity becomes a factor. Presently, a common educational practice of classifying children for intervention services is through categorical or disorder based approaches (Hardman et al., 1996). For purposes of intervention research, participants are divided into discrete groups based on individual characteristics or disorders, usually based on the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders - Fourth Edition (DSM-IV). The selection of treatment based upon a categorical approach is based on the assumption that children will respond differentially to treatments contingent on their diagnosis. For example, it is assumed that children with Oppositional Defiant Disorder have different characteristics and instructional needs than children with Attention Deficit Disorders or Conduct Disorders. According to a categorical approach, grouping each of these children under their respective labels would be most functional in meeting their needs (Hardman et al., 1996). Thus, the selection of target populations (usually homogenous groups) 25 for intervention research has the dual intent of maximizing treatment gains as well as controlling for any extraneous variables. Several other researchers have argued that categorical approaches are neither necessary nor beneficial (Hardman et al., 1996). They argue that there is no need to separate children by categorical labels, traditional categorizing approaches result in unnecessary separation, and categorizing can be an expensive label. Considerable debate exists among proponents of cross- categorical approaches and categorical systems (Hardman et al., 1996). Cross- categorical approaches do not use the traditional categories in defining and classifying children with Ieaming and behavior disorders but do identify children according to the level of severity of their difficulties (Hardman et al., 1996, p. 59). When serious consideration is given to the parameters or state of the art in the current classification system, DSM-IV, then a categorical approach may not be the most functional for intervention research with disruptive behavior disorders. An example of a disruptive behavior disorders category which may have lost its utility as a research label is Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD). The literature on Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder is replete with hard core research, interactive clinical research (actuarial science), popular psychology, and anecdotal accounts. It should be noted that a historical overview of attention deficit disorders indicates several diagnostic labels and descriptions of children with behaviors consistent with ADHD prior to the publication of the first DSM (Lerner & Lerner, 1991). As cited in Lerner and Lerner (1991), these categories included Brain Damage Syndrome (Werner & 26 Straus, 1941; 1947) and Minimal Brain Dysfunction (Clements & Peters, 1962) despite lack of evidence that these children had suffered brain damage. Even earlier, near the end of the nineteenth century, Heinrich Hoffman, a German physician and poet described children with ADHD characteristics as “Fidgety Phils” and George Stillman, an English Physician, described these children as defective in control of behavior and inhibition (Barkley, 1992). Despite these descriptions, it was not until the second DSM in which ADHD was recognized (Table 7). This historical summary supports the observation that the various Diagnostic and Statistical Manuals of Mental Disorders (DSMs) demonstrate a lag in research conceptualizations; furthermore, variables and factors other than rigorous scientific experimentation are instrumental in decisions regarding the creation and publication of these diagnostic manuals (Barkley, 1993; Pelham, 1996; Tannock, 1998). As a result, it is posited that specific intervention strategies should not be based on a generic diagnosis of ADHD. With the publication of every Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM), changes to the Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) category have involved specific definitional criteria and nomenclature (Table 7). In fact, there have occurred major changes regarding what the criteria are at either a sub-symptom level or addressing the category of ADHD as a whole. The debate has been about how best to weight the symptoms, how best to combine them, and so on. Most recently, diagnosis of ADHD changed drastically from DSM-lll-R to DSM-IV. A number of changes had been recommended for inclusion in the final 27 Table 7 Summaries of DSM Nomenclatures and TheoreticalfViews of ADHD DSM Nomenclature Proposed but Rejected Nomenclature Theoretical Era DSM No recognition NA* None DSM-ll Hyperkinetic Kyperkinetic Reaction Hyperactivity (1968) Reaction of Childhood Motor DSM-III ADD / + H Attention Deficit Disorder Attention (1980) ADD / - H DSM-lll- ADHD ID (Impulse Control) Motivational (1987) U-ADHD HID (Hyperactive Impulse response to Disorder) rewards DSM-IV ADHD-IN EFD (Executive Function Self Regulation (1994) ADHD-HI Disorder) Disinhibition ADHD-Co Executive Dysfunction DSM-V NA SRD + IN Self Regulation (2001) SRD + HI With lnattention Self Regulation with Hyperactivity- lmpulsivity 28 draft of DSM-IV by the Committee on the Disruptive Behavior Disorders (Barkley, 1993). Empirical field trials and expert consultations provided the basis for the recommended changes (McBumett et al., 1993). Several studies provide detailed descriptions of the field trial process for the DSM-IV (McBumett et al., 1993; Power & DuPaul,1996). Symptom items were field tested on both normal and clinic referred children at 10 or 11 sites across the United States, depending on the source (McBumett et al., 1993 and Power & DuPaul, 1996, respectively). Researchers used predictive power statistics to determine diagnostic utility (Power & DuPaul, 1996). Experimental items which did not demonstrate sufficient level of predictive power were excluded from the diagnostic criteria. Other criteria which remained uncertain, required further analyses from the field trial data set (Barkley, 1993). The entire Child and Adolescent Categories Work Group and other APA governance structures passed approval on the majority of recommendations by the committee. Other additional diagnostic criteria were recommended by committee members but did not pass approval and were not incorporated in the ensuing DSM (Barkley, 1993). These included the following: adjust DSM-lll-R cutoff for age symptoms exceed 93 °/o of same age normal children onset by 6 years of age (arguable) duration of symptoms of 12 months symptoms occur across many tasks and situations demanding persistence if IQ is below 85, use mental age norms in 1. above (adapted from Barkley, 1993) 9959539 9’ 29 One final recommendation involved re-instating the Attention Deficit Disorder (ADD) category to describe those children previously labeled ADD without Hyperactivity (DSM-Ill) or Undifferentiated ADD (DSM-lll-R). The final published DSM-lV (1994) contains two clusters or lists of symptoms to focus on: 1) lnattention symptoms and 2) lmpulsivity-Hyperactivity Symptoms conceptualized as one cluster (Pelham, 1996; Tannock, 1998). Several lnattention symptoms appear to be taken directly from the Connors Rating Scale. Six symptoms from this list are part of the criteria for meeting diagnosis of ADHD - predominantly lnattentive (ADHD-IN). Similarly, six symptoms from the lmpulsivity-Hyperactivity Symptoms lists are required to meet diagnosis of ADHD-predominantly Hyperactive-Impulsive (ADHD-HI). Research has demonstrated that the impulsivity and hyperactivity tend to cluster together providing an impetus to group the symptoms under one list in the DSM-IV (Power & DuPaul, 1996). DSM-IV is the first time, Hyperactivity- lmpulsivity is put in as a separate category. When factor analysis research is done, symptoms relating to hyperactivity do not separate out from those indicating impulsivity. Research from the DSM-IV field trials has shown that impulsivity and hyperactivity load together to form a single behavioral dimension (Applegate et al., 1997). All but one of the studies reviewed by McBumett et al. (1993) found a two factor structure: inattention and hyperactivity/impulsivity. Hinshaw (1994) in his review also supports the notion of two stable dimensions: inattention/disorganization and impulsivity/hyperactivity. 30 The DSM-IV conceptualizations of ADHD are based on the belief that these two clusters of symptoms are distinct in terms of their etiology, in their clinical course, prognosis, and response to treatment. Researchers argue that this belief in distinct subtypes, is as of yet still unfounded by empirical data, but is the very premise upon which the DSM-IV is based (Power & DuPaul, 1996; Tannock, 1998). Furthermore, the DSM-Ill category of ADHD without Hyperactivity cannot be conceptualized as the same as the DSM-IV category of ADHD-Predominantly lnattentive type. The former incorporated symptoms related to impulsivity while the latter does not. Currently, there is a conceptualization or belief that three distinct types of children exist which can all receive this global label of ADHD: ADHD-IN and ADHD-HI as mentioned above as well as ADHD - Combined type (ADHD-CO). Other researchers argue that differences in meeting the 7 year old age of onset criteria among subtypes (ADHD-IN, 43%; ADHD-HI, 100%; & ADHD-CO, 18%) support the validity of these three distinct types of children (Applegate et al., 1997). These latter results, however, raise the question about the validity of the age of onset of impairment criteria rather than an age of onset of symptoms criteria. As a result of the somewhat arbitrary judgements and beliefs, the DSM- IV also contains several additional criteria changes which have the potential to pose considerable problems. First is the criteria that symptoms have to be present before the age of 7. Tannock (1998) found a low reliability of recall among parents of ADHD children. Only 33 % of parents were able to recall the age of onset one year after initial clinic visit. However, Green et al. (1991) found 31 recall of age of onset of ADHD behavior by the mother as stable. While age of onset is a historically based requirement, there has been little consistent empirical evidence to support this criterion. First, age of onset is difficult to establish reliably through retrospective studies and second, age of onset makes little difference in the reliability of the diagnosis if it is not required (Barkley, 1993; Tannock, 1998). This age cutoff for the onset of ADHD symptoms appear to be a continuing source of disagreement among researchers. Researchers have previously argued to keep the diagnostic cutoff at age 7 (Green et al., 1991), lowering the cutoff to age 6 (Barkley, 1993), and having no cutoff at all (Power & DuPaul, 1996). A developmentally appropriate cutoff criterion is proposed because a marked decline in hyperactive-impulsive behaviors with age, has been found in the general population (Barkley, 1993). Barkley (1993) argued that a fixed cut off score is not logical in populations with declining baselines. This fixed criterion is problematic because clinicians are likely to over diagnose preschool children and under-diagnose adolescents and adults. From this perspective, arguments that people outgrow the disorder can be viewed as artifacts of having a fixed cutoff score in the diagnostic criteria. This issue has become one of the criticisms of the DSM-IV, i.e., it fails to account for developmental changes in ADHD presentation (Power & DuPaul, 1996). Requiring the presence of symptoms in two or more settings (e.g. at school, work, and/or home) presents tremendous difficulties for clinicians (Power & DuPaul, 1996; Tannock, 1998). These may include problems with 32 how to operationalize this requirement in practice, how clinicians know how to select which two settings and informants, and how clinicians decide what constitutes a setting (e.g., are the school playground and school classroom two different settings or do they constitute one setting?) Setting and informant can easily become confounded in any diagnostic decisions. Tannock (1998) found that there is major discrepancy or disagreement among informants especially between parents and teachers. It the teacher states one thing and parents state something else who is the more credible source, particularly if inattention and hyperactive impulsivity are distinct problems. Similarly, August and Garfinkel (1993) reported that requiring two or more settings (home and school) or two corroborating sources may be too restrictive. Not all researchers have arrived at the same conclusion. Biederman et al. (1993) concluded that although correlation between parent and teacher ratings were low to moderate, parent ratings were corroborated by teacher reports using “broad” teacher diagnoses of ADHD. The DSM-IV stipulates that problems in impairment must be present in social and/or academic settings in order to meet criteria (Power & DuPaul, 1996). This leaves room for considerable interpretation. In fact, many clinicians report paying homage to the DSMs, following the criteria to some degree, and correcting for errors and doing an even better job by adding their own idiosyncratic criteria (Applegate et al., 1997). Barkley (1997) recommends that clinicians locate evidence that the symptoms existed in several settings but not insisting on agreement between parents and teachers in order to diagnosis. These elements of clinical 33 judgement create a potentially unreliable diagnostic process (Power & DuPaul, 1996). According to the DSM-IV, the prevalence of ADHD is estimated at 3% to 5% among school age children (APA, 1994). According to its predecessor, the DSM-lll-R, the prevalence of ADHD was described as common and as occurring in as many as 3% of school age children (APA, 1987). Surprisingly, this increase suggests that the prevalence of children diagnosed with ADHD is higher when requirements stipulate two settings. However, the criteria for ADHD also stipulated the presence of 8 symptoms as opposed to 6 in the DSM-lll-R and DSM-IV, respectively. The field trial researchers also recommended many more symptoms for the DSM-IV but symptoms were dropped due to nonscientific rationale. An anecdotal account revealed that one of the higher ups in the APA governing body decided that there were too many symptoms and wanted some dropped (Barkley, 1993; Pelham, 1996). For political reasons, the APA governing body chose two smaller symptom lists, one with hyperactive and impulsive symptoms congregated together and one with inattention symptoms together and reduced the list with the claim that clinicians would not be able to remember the larger lists (Barkley, 1993). The changes appear scientifically questionable since shortening an item list tends to weaken reliability which in turn weakens the validity of the diagnostic criteria. The Committee had originally planned that children who met criteria on the Hyperactive Impulsive would be called ADHD and children who met criteria on the lnattentive list would be ADD. The DSM-IV governance bodies changed this recommendation so that all children who meet 34 any criteria are labeled as ADHD but with one of three trailing clauses: Predominantly lnattentive, Predominantly Hyperactive-Impulsive, or Predominantly Combined Type. Barkley (1993) viewed this change as regressive scientifically in that it was a throw back to DSM-Ill where ADD and ADHD were viewed as sub-types of a common problem, i.e., they were viewed as the same disorder. Barkley (1993) did not view ADD as a disruptive behavior disorder and did not even want to include ADD in the Disruptive behavior disorders section of the DSM. Thus, according to Barkley (1993) DSM-IV obscured where it could have clarified. Certainly, given an analysis of some of the field trial studies specifically regarding symptoms (Applegate et al., 1997), it becomes apparent that research may not be the determining factor in devising diagnostic manuals. Other researchers have found evidence consistent with the notion that ADD and ADHD are separate disorders (August & Garfinkel, 1993). Goodyear and Hynd (1992) in an extensive review of the literature suggested that ADHD/H and ADHD/ WO present with distinct behavioral and neuropsychological differences that distinguish them in clinical presentation, etiology, and possibly intervention and that the two groups may constitute two separate disorders. Later in the same article, they qualified this suggestion and concluded that their review does not permit a categorical answer to the question of the existence of ADD subtypes. Goodman and Poillion (1992) synthesized 48 articles and books to examine the agreement regarding 35 characteristics and cause of ADHD. They found 69 characteristics and 38 causes. Consistent with the notion of separate disorders are several growing clinical beliefs which are unsubstantiated by conclusive empirical data. One belief is that the risk factor lies with the Hyperactive-Impulsive Type and that lnattentive types are less severely impaired (Tannock, 1998). The implications are severe; if clinicians believe that lnattentive type children have less severe problems, then it is likely that the lnattentive types will receive less treatment or intervention (Tannock, 1998). Other related general beliefs are that 1) lnattentive type are more likely to have more anxiety, depressive, and reading disorders and 2) The hyperactive-impulsive type are more likely to have more oppositional defiant disorder, conduct disorder related problems. Tannock (1998) found that hyperactive impulsive children are more likely to have conduct disorder but oppositional defiant disorder did not cluster together. Morgan et al. (1996) found that ADHD/Combined type children are perceived by their parents to display significantly more externalizing, delinquent, and aggressive behavior than ADHD/lnattentive type. Findings from studies suggest that ADHD/Hyperactive-lmpulsive types are at greater risk for antisocial behavior but no longitudinal studies have been conducted to date (Power & DuPaul, 1996). One earlier research study identified three different subtypes of ADHD: ADD without hyperactivity; ADD with hyperactivity; and ADD with hyperactivity and aggression, ADDHA, (Dykman & Ackerman, 1993). Other research found no significant difference between ADHD subtypes and 36 rate of affective or anxiety disorder co-diagnosis (Morgan et al., 1996). Other subtypes have been proposed in the literature including ADHD with and without aggression, and ADHD with learning disabilities (Power & DuPaul, 1996). However, ADHD with and without hyperactivity children have similar rates of Ieaming disabilities when reading and math disabilities are grouped together (Barkley, 1990; Morgan et al., 1996). The latest ICD also classifies a hyperkinetic-conduct disorder subtype. Power and DuPaul (1996) recommend continuing to treat these subtypes as co-morbid disorders rather than distinct subtypes. For a nomenclature to be effective, common characteristics must be attributed to the system to be known and agreed upon by all who use the system. Thus, the entire diagnostic and nomenclature issue of ADHD is not only complicated by the heterogeneity of ADHD but also by the prevalence of comorbidity. This has huge implications for future research directions but especially for current diagnosis and treatment. Currently, there are no empirical studies which examine ADHD subtypes and differential response to various treatments including pharmacological intervention, contingency management, and modification to academic instruction (Power & DuPaul, 1996). Yet researchers have made recommendations based on subtypes: In terms of intervention, the findings indicate that with combined type children, it is important to identify and treat aggressive, oppositional, or delinquent behavior and/or to take a preventative approach (Morgan et al.1996) 37 Power and DuPaul (1996) suggested that the DSM-IV diagnostic criteria should be useful for predicting effective lists of treatment for children diagnosed with ADHD. Recommended treatments for ADHD include parent training, family therapy, adaptation in school environment, pharmaceutical, behavior intervention in the classroom and playground, and involvement in parent support groups (Power & DuPaul, 1996). However, the utility of diagnosis for treatment is very general and therefore limited in being helpful for designing behavioral and instructional intervention (Power & DuPaul, 1996). Goodman and Poillion, (1992) also held the view that diagnosis has limited value for treatment because of the uncertainty of the nature of ADHD. Pelham (1996) also presents a strong argument for why clinicians should place less emphasis on diagnosis for attention deficit disorders and nomenclature. Because the criteria change all of the time, practicing clinicians should not be overly concerned or devote inordinate amounts of time to diagnosis (Pelham, 1996). It is probable that the next DSM (either DSM-lV-R or DSM-V) will have a different list of symptoms, or different way of grouping them, and a different way of conceptualizing the disorder. Throughout the years, they have all been the same children, they all have impairment, and they have all had problems that required treatment. It would be a rare clinician who assessed children, found them just short of meeting the number of symptoms, and sent them on their way. Regardless of whether the child needs 12 symptoms and only meets 10 for criteria, he still has a problem which likely requires some form of intervention. As a result, Pelham (1996) recommends 38 establishing impairment and designing treatment based on impairment. However, any decisions regarding impairment also require a cut-off score. Barkley (1993) argues that how professionals conceptualize the disorder will affect how clients will be treated. This is problematic considering that ADHD has been conceptualized from various perspectives for decades. Given the lack of consensus in the field, treatments cannot be tailored to ADHD. Thus, an argument can be made that the ensuring recommendations for multi-modal treatments for children with ADHD can be viewed as a blitz treatment to cover all contingencies. The anecdotal accounts from various sources (Barkley, 1994; Pelham, 1997; Tannock, 1998) suggest that specific criteria that are a part of diagnosis in the DSMs vary tremendously according to many different dimensions or parameters of which research may be a relatively small part. Basically, a treatment committee put together the diagnostic criteria and governing bodies made the final decisions. It was originally predicted that using the DSM-Field Trial data would improve the reliability, internal consistency, and clarify the association of ADHD with functional impairment (McBumett et al., 1993). Regarding ADHD in particular, the DSM continues to be criticized for being far from fully reliable, specific, or valid (Hinshaw, 1994). “The clinical field has long grappled with issues of classification, diagnosis, underlying patho-physiology, associated difficulties, and treatment of youth with this syndrome" (McBumett et al., 1993). Given the shifting nomenclature, diagnostic criteria, and theoretical models of ADHD presented above, one can ask the question, how can 39 clinicians decide on effective treatment based on a diagnosis. Inter-clinician agreement on what the problem is constitutes a prerequisite to linking diagnosis and treatment. This categorical — treatment issue with ADHD is further complicated by the prevalence of comorbidity with other disruptive behavior disorders as well as other DSM-IV disorders. The research literature is converging to the view that Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder is likely a neurodevelopmentally disorder (Barkley, 1993; 1995, 1997). However, the developmental progression of ADHD into other disruptive behaviors disorders has been viewed within a behaviorist model. Large percentages of children with ADHD (35 to 60% ) develop Oppositional Defiant Disorder (ODD) by 7 years of age and a large percentage (30 to 50%) will eventually meet criteria for Conduct Disorders (CD) (Whalen & Henker, 1998). Predictors of development into ODD behaviors were found to be parental consistency and predictability. Predictors of development into CD were related to parental monitoring and tracking. These predictors are related to parent characteristics. It is also noted that a substantial percentage (15 to 25%) eventually develop antisocial personality into adulthood (Whalen & Henker, 1998). Finally, it has been reported that the nature or type of children’s disruptive behavior disorders is not predictive of parent training outcomes (Ruma et al., 1996). Rather the severity of the disorder is a more reliable predictor. One of the parameters of cross categorical approaches, characterize children according to the severity of their condition (e.g., mild, moderate, or 40 severe). To objectively evaluate the proposition that PMT is suitable to all disruptive disorders, Table 8 summarizes similarities on various dimensions among children with ADHD, CD, and ODD, as well as differences (Anastopoulos, Barkley, & Sheldon, 1996; Barkley, 1998; Clarizio, 1996; Frick, 1998; Hinshaw & Anderson, 1996; Kazdin, 1996; Marx & Gross, 1998; McMahon & Wells, 1998; Whalen & Henker, 1998). Brestan and Eyberg (1998) describe PMT as having one of the strongest bases in controlled outcome research for children with behavior disorders. Furthermore, research has documented PMT’s efficacy across a wide age range of children with various kinds of behavior problems (Brestan & Eyberg, 1998). Thus, these findings provide support for the view that cross-categorical approaches can be used in PMT intervention research. PREDICTORS IN PARENT TRAINING INTERVENTIONS Predictors of outcomes in PMT can be grouped according to two broad categories which may not necessarily be mutually exclusive: 1) traditionally examined predictors and, more recently, 2) parent cognitions. The first include primarily demographic types of predictors as well as child characteristics. The second, parent cognitions, may include attitudes, beliefs, and perspectives as well as inner motivational resources. Traditionally Examined Outcomes And Predictors Outcome research has consistently confirmed the efficacy of parent training as a treatment for changing behavior problems in children (Ruma et al., 1996). Considerable research has also been conducted throughout the years 41 Table 8 A Comparison of Disruptive Behavior Di_sorders Behavior Disorder Dimension AD/HD ODD CD CARDINAL SYPTOMS Core hyperactive-impulsive defiant aggressive (disinhibition) irritable antisocial inattention oppositional infliction of pain severity fluctuates initiating fights according to context denial of others' rights persistent not transient Number of Symptoms 6/18 4/8 3/ 1 5 EPIDEMIOLOGICAL Subtypes Predominantly lnattentive none Childhood-Onset Predominantly Hyperactive- Adolescent-Onset Impulsive Predominantly- Combined Prevalence 2 % to 6.3 % 5 to 7% 6 to 16% males 2 to 16% 2 to 9 % females Age of Onset <3years, PHI before 8 years as early as 5 to 6 years 5 to 7 years, Pl usually childhood or adoles. Sex ratios 3:1 community samples aprox. 1:1 2:1 to 4:1 (male to female) 6:1 to 9:1 clinic samples ASSOCIATED CHARACTERISTICS Social Problems yes yes yes Medical Risks yes na na Conscience Delays no no yes, deficient . (moral reasoning) normal lack empathy, guilt Voluntary control no yes yes over behavior uninhibited, minimal control calculated, planful Attachment relations typically poor typically poor poor quality Academic Problems yes yes yes 42 Con't Table 8 ETIOLOGIES Heredity Influence 10 to 35% no yes Neurobehavioral yes negative yes Patterns strong evidence temperament (strong with early onset) prefrontal lobes cognitive characteristics Dietary Contributions unsubstantiated na na Environmental Toxinsyes no no Psychosocial lnfluencyes (on severity) yes (quality of yes (strong with adolescent Contextual parent-child onset) interactions) DEVELOPMENTAL TRAJECTORIES/ PATHWAYS age change in yes no yes cardinal symptoms maximal minimal small influence on pathwaysubtype parent subtype consistency parent monitoring & tracking parent influence on minimal consistency monitoring symptom progress compensatory strategies predictability tracking re: symptoms indiscriminant parenting characterization heterotypic continuity highly stable heterotypic continuity INTERVENTIONS Punishment Need responsive to external need for changeeward dominant learning in Treatment controls in coercive style interactions PMT efficicacious efficacious efficacious Pharmacotherapy methylphenidate no specific no specific pharma- antidepressants pharmaco- cotherapy for core therapy for clonidine for aggression core lithium for aggression and symptoms explosiveness Cognitive-Problem yes na yes Solving promising efficacious Choice of target self control compliance covert (stealing, lying, behaviors self regulation firesetting) overt (bullying, compliance) 43 examining predictors of successful and unsuccessful outcomes in parent training programs with various disruptive behavior populations. The outcomes have included a broad range of measures across studies. The predictors have primarily included parent depression, marital status and adjustment, social class, number of negative life experiences, maternal insularity, age, and severity of disorders, (Ruma et al., 1996; Webster-Stratton & Hammond, 1990). There has been considerable interest in how and why some parent training programs work for some parents and not others. Several years ago, Webster-Stratton (1985) demonstrated that depending on definition of treatment outcome criterion, different clusters of predictors of treatment will have varying success or failures. These treatment outcome criteria can be grouped into three broad categories: a) changes in child behavior; b) parent engagement; and, c) parent satisfaction. Many studies have measured childhood outcomes as perceived by parents on rating scales. Some studies have examined outcomes of child behavior through direct observation in home and school. Other studies have examined parental outcomes and changes in parent’s behavior. One study reported changes in parent attitude; however, examination of the study revealed that parental attitude was measured through having the parent rate the child’s behavior on the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) (Webster-Stratton, 1990). The CBCL appears to have set the standard as an outcome measure of changes in child behavior. 44 A second category of outcome measures involves parent satisfaction. Consumer satisfaction has become an essential component of outcome evaluation of psychological treatments (including parent training), due to the recent changes in mental health delivery, funding, and emphasis on treatment effectiveness (Brestan, Jacobs, Rayfield, & Eyberg, 1999) as well as managed care organization preference (Bilbrey & Bilbrey, 1995, July/August). Furthermore, (Brestan et al., 1999) view standardized measures of treatment satisfaction with child treatments, as a priority. Parent satisfaction would be defined as the extent to which parents liked the PMT process and outcomes which include the format, techniques, and effects of treatment (Brestan et al., 1999). Webster-Stratton (1993) reports that Patterson's PMT treatment approach, Forehand and Mahon’s approach, and Webster-Stratton’s programs have been extensively evaluated and all have had high reports of parent satisfaction. Outcomes have also been assessed through examination of parental engagement. Several indexes of parental engagement have included premature drop out and behavioral signs in training which may include sporadic attendance, missed and late appointments, last minute cancellations, skipped sessions without cancellation, and failure to complete homework (Prinz & Miller, 1996). Other signs of inadequate parental engagement include quality of participation in sessions such as low level of interaction, complaints or arguments, giving up on strategies after one attempt, and refusing to role play (Prinz & Miller, 1996). 45 Holden et al. (1990) describe three related criteria in assessing parent engagement outcomes in parent training programs: 1)the proportion of clients who successfully complete training; 2) the magnitude of client’s behavioral change; and, 3) the length of time needed to bring about change. Previous researchers have described outcomes according to two levels: 1) those clients who terminate treatment prematurely and drop out; and, 2) those clients who stay in treatment but fail to demonstrate positive change (Webster-Stratton, 1985). Holden et al. (1990) break these levels down further in their description of a “continuum of effectiveness”. At one end, certain clients may progress rapidly and show improvement (Holden et al., 1990). In the middle of the continuum, clients may experience difficulty in attaining treatment goals. They complete training and take longer to demonstrate progress but only after training has been completed (Holden et al., 1990). It is probable that these middle clients would present as clients at the opposite end of the continuum unless long term outcomes are evaluated. At this opposite end, clients may drop out from training and for a variety of reasons are least likely to benefit from training. Several studies have noted significant changes in parent report measures but nonsignificant changes in child behavior data (Webster-Stratton, 1985). Several studies have also reported that immediate post treatment gains were not maintained on follow up (Webster-Stratton, 1990). Kazdin (1997) summarized the conclusions of several outcome studies as follows: 1) parent management training outcomes as measured by parent and teacher reports of deviant behavior, direct home and school observations, and institutional records (e.g., 46 truancy, police contact, arrests) indicate significant improvement; 2) the magnitude of changes place children with conduct disordered within normal ranges of behavior in comparison to non-referred same sex, age peers; 3) long term treatment gains from 1 to 3 years have been maintained; 4) studies include some sparse data on “incidental improvements” such as in marital satisfaction, maternal psychopathology (depression), family cohesion, and sibling behavior which were not a direct focus of training. The most commonly studied predictors of parent training outcomes, henceforth called traditionally examined predictors (TEPs), include single parent families (SPFs), socioeconomic status (SES), parent psychopathology (especially, maternal depression), number of psychosocial stressors, and severity of the child’s behavior problem. Single Parent Families (SPF) Treatment failure has also been related to marital adjustment (Webster- Stratton & Hammond, 1990). Specifically, single parent families are more likely to be non-responsive to treatment or drop out of treatment (Webster-Stratton, 1985; Wester-Stratton & Hammond, 1990). In contrast, intact families are more successful in maintaining treatment effects over time (Webster-Stratton & Hammond, 1990). No mention was made as to the nature of the marital relationship and whether or not this would have an impact in parent training. The Webster-Stratton and Hammond (1990) study examined multiple predictor measures with a large sample of parents at post treatment and at one year follow up. They found that marital status and SES were the strongest 47 predictors of maternal behavior with their children post-treatment while marital status alone was the strongest predictor of paternal behavior (Webster-Stratton & Hammond, 1990). In contrast, Holden et al. (1990) found that marital status was not found to be predictive of parent training outcomes. In their case, outcome was defined as completion of the program. Marital status appears to be a variable worthy of examination. Single- parent families are a growing segment of the US. population (25%) and Canadian population (20%) and usually headed by mothers (Statistics Canada, 1992; US. Bureau of the Census, 1994). Socioeconomic Status (SE8) Socioeconomic disadvantage has been defined by low income, low education, single parent status, poor area of residence, large number of children in the family, and referral to the clinic by outside agency (Webster-Stratton, 1985; Wester-Stratton & Hammond, 1990). Family SES has been consistently reported to be related to parent training outcome. Low SES parents were more likely to drop out of parent training programs (Kazdin & Wassell, 1999; Webster- Stratton & Hammond, 1990) or do more poorly in parent training in that they were generally less successful in making positive behavioral changes (Holden et al., 1990; Kazdin & Wassell, 1999). These results were found at both immediate post-treatment and long term post treatment. An earlier summary of the research literature indicated that SES was an important predictor for outcome criteria, i.e., the more disadvantaged the family (single, low income, and low education), the less likely it was to benefit from parent training (Webster-Stratton, 1985). 48 On the other hand, advantaged SES can also be viewed as predictive of poor outcomes in terms of missed appointments and dropouts. Financially secure families may be overly committed to individual activities (e.g. volunteering or church) and recreational activities (sports or clubs) (Prinz & Miller, 1996). Engagement in these other activities can lead to inflexible or infrequent scheduling of sessions (Prinz & Miller, 1996). Also, families with extensive other activities may have disorganized lifestyles which would make it difficult to practice new strategies learned in parent training (Prinz & Miller, 1996). Parent Psychopathology Studies have suggested that pretreatment levels of maternal depression were significantly related to treatment failure or relapses (Kazdin & Wassell, 1999; Webster-Stratton & Hammond, 1990). Webster-Stratton and Hammond (1990) also found that paternal depression was just as predictive of reports on child adjustment. Not all studies have found maternal depression to be a predictor of parent training outcomes. One study found that maternal depression as measured by the Beck Depression Inventory, was not a predictor in parent training outcomes (Webster-Stratton, 1985). It was hypothesized that this may have been the result of maternal depression decreasing significantly from pre- to post-treatment assessment. Similarly, Frankel and Simmons (1992) did not find evidence to support the notion that maternal depression is a predictor of parent training outcomes. They found that the only parent personality variable which was related to parent training outcomes was psychotic distortions. However, it was noted that 49 parents endorsed scale items which reflected helplessness and negativity rather than psychotic processes (Frankel & Simmons, 1992). Helplessness and negativity are also consistent with depressive affect. These parents tended to drop out of treatment (Frankel & Simmons, 1992). Psychosocial Stressors Ecological variables have been found to have a significant contribution to parent training outcomes. For example, stressful life experiences make it difficult to find time to do assignments, participate in parent training, and follow through with suggestions (Kazdin & Wassell, 1999; Webster-Stratton & Herbert, 1993). Also, the degree of life crisis and environmental stresses would be significantly related to a family's ability to maintain treatment effects (Webster-Stratton, 1985). Situational demands and constraints have an influence on behavioral outcomes in treatment. Prinz and Miller (1996) discussed the research in this area and concluded that a multitude of stressors has an adverse impact on training outcomes including parental engagement in therapy, reduced treatment motivation and participation, as well as the parent child relationship. Parents with a multitude of environmental and personal stressors are more likely to over react to daily hassles (car repairs, sick family member) (Prinz & Miller, 1996). Parents may rationalize and attribute drop outs and missed appointments to these types of situational obstacles (Prinz & Miller, 1996). Webster-Stratton (1985) hypothesized that an interaction between types of life experiences and the availability of social supports may be predictive of parent training outcomes. Wahler’s early seminal research found that single mothers 50 who have contact with other people outside the home have much more treatment success than mothers who are socially isolated (Dumas & Wahler, 1983). Maternal insularity increases the probability of parent training program failure (Wester-Stratton & Hammond, 1990). Other researchers have found that social insularity may interact with other stressors to affect parenting styles (Dadds 8 McHugh, 1992). High levels of social support was found to be predictive of how parents responded to treatment, specifically mothers without support failed to benefit from traditional parent training (Dadds & McHugh, 1992). However, when access to social support systems was made a part of the treatment, mothers without their own supports still failed to benefit (Dadds & McHugh, 1992). Severity of Child Behavior Studies have discussed the limited effects of parent training for adolescents (Ruma et al., 1996). Ruma et al. (1966) used archival data to test for age effects on the outcomes of parent group training. They found that the best predictor of parent training outcome was the severity of the child’s problems and concluded that severity of the behavior problem was responsible for the age effects in parent training (Ruma et al., 1996). This study, however, was limited in that they had no control or comparison group, follow-up data was not available, long term effects were not noted, outcome measures were limited to one global rating of psychopathology among children (the Child Behavior Checklist), and one informant (mothers) provided the rating (Ruma et al., 1996). However, other researchers have also found that parents of children with greater numbers of presenting problems were less likely to complete their parent 51 training program (Holden et al., 1990; Kazdin & Wassell, 1999). Kazdin (1997) reported that more severe antisocial behavior and comorbidity are predictive of negative outcomes to parent management training. Other Predictors Webster-Stratton (1993) summarized the research on predictors and concluded fewer treatment gains with the following parent and family characteristics: marital distress, spousal abuse, lack of a supportive partner, maternal depression, poor problem solving skills, high life stress, and SES disadvantages coupled with lack of social support. Bank et al. (1991) studied time in treatment as a predictor and offense rates among chronic delinquents as the outcome variables. They found a significant treatment by time effect with greater reduced rates of serious crimes with time in therapy (Bank et al., 1991). Holden et al. (1990) found that ethnic minorities were less likely to complete parent training. Failure to Find Consistent Predictors Based on the review of studies examining predictors, there is no single consistent predictor of treatment success or failure (Table 9). For example, evidence from the Carolina Early Intervention Program indicated that children demonstrated benefit regardless of parent (mother in this case) intelligence, age, education, or type of family (Ramey et al., 1988). The apparent failure to find consistent predictive outcomes based on variables identified in the literature may be due to several limitations in the research literature. 52 «00:0 000: . 3000500.: «0005090 50 0000 :0 028620 00:29:00 .0 02.2005 00:29:00 :0 026690 00:99:00 :0 02306000 :00 .0958: 0_ 0.0: 3:985 .0800 wmw 30. 00050.83 0.050 0068.000 .mEEmE mam.» 3:00.00 2050 200005 5600000030 0390 3:00.. 900305.00 200000 DO 000 0000=0E00000 w 02000030 00. 00:90: :8: :95»; w 0090000 .93. 63000.2 £03 88: 29:05: a 8:20.868; 83: 090800 a .0530... .0020: 0858; mmm 30. 000500000 060$ 00 Ewe:_0m_m-000 020000000 .mEcfiE 006000000 .mEQmE 000 38: 000005.568; 093090006 0_E000000_0om 900000 060?. 800030 “000000.: 000. a 3:235 _m_0om 0030000 38: 00.00; a. 8an 000620000 0050096 Esmom £22005 00:053.”. >036 @550; Ewan. 0_ m00«0_000d&0_0:0|0xw 863w :0 EMEEzw m 298. 53 0000000 0: 00:00: >0 00000300. 0000000 50328 200 .o 02.285 00.0090. 00.000 00.0850 006.0000 3:0>00 0.30 090 00 0200600 050.090 U.0 028.0000 00:00.0:000 30. 0.00050 00 00.0.0000 .000:_00_0-000 50 080 00 00:00 .0090 006600 0:00:00 3000 0. 05:3 32050000000 E0000 mum 00000000 5030000 00 3:0>00 0.00000: 5.3 0.0000:0_00 .0000>0_00 00.000000 00:08:00 .0000 0030000 00 3:0>00 0030000 0:00 00 3:0>00 00000000 .0.000000>00 000 :00 00000000 060000000 00.000000: 00.00 000.090.: 0:000>00 000000 .0000 30. Do _0_000:00 02000500 .000:_00000 .0800 -:00 0.000 0.00.0000 0030000 0000000 00 5. x0: .0 00 0.00.000 0030000 .0 :00__0E00 .000 0000000 .000: 020002, 0 :6me that >0_>>o._0 w 000:0... .06me :00: 060v. .000: 8859: 0 .9030 .0050 .000: .002 0 2:0 .000: 0090... 0 000000000000; .000: 00055.0 0 00.00.”. .000: 00:00.2 0 00000 m 0.00... 3000 54 In a review of parent training research, Rogers Wiese (1992) discussed the shortcomings in research methodology which had been addressed by previous reviews as follows: 1) an over-reliance on reporting data about child behavior changes rather than specific parent behavior changes; 2) inconsistent use of multiple observers; 3) failure to report inter-rater reliability with the former; 4) failure to use multiple outcome measures; 5) little assessment of generalization of treatment effects; and 6) limited, if any, follow up data (Moreland et al., 1982; Rogers Wiese 8. Kramer, 1988). Subsequent research appears to have addressed many of these methodology shortcomings. Notably, later parent training research shows an upward trend in collecting parent change data, increased multiple outcome measures, and collection of generalization data (Wiese, 1992). Group studies in parent research also contained several methodological limitations. These included a failure to gather or include follow up data, failure to include control groups, minimal collection of treatment integrity data, and fairly limited reporting of assessment and treatment protocols (Wiese, 1992). These undermined the internal and external validity of the group results and thus make it impossible to determine if the intervention is responsible for changes and if changes generalize over time, settings, and people (Wiese, 1992). Several researchers have Webster-Stratton and Hammond (1990) described several confounding issues in many parent training studies as follows: 1. examined only one isolated predictor variable at a time, or combined predictors in a single index score making it difficult to determine effects of individual predictors (Webster-Stratton 8. Hammond, 1990) 2. lack of a standard method to assess treatment success or “clinical 55 significance" (Webster-Stratton 8. Hammond, 1990) 3. almost exclusive reliance on single not multiple outcome measures such as maternal information, neglecting paternal variables (Webster-Stratton 8. Hammond, 1990) and ignoring parent and child (Tucker et al., 1998) 4. using small homogeneous samples, lack of control groups at long term follow up, lack of random assignment, and wide variability in training programs (Tucker et al., 1998). Two specific examples of methodology shortcomings involve the child’s age as a predictor and the selection of outcome measures. While age has been discussed as a predictor of outcomes, studies have also seldom controlled for different age groups, younger and older children, whose parents are involved in training (Ruma et al., 1996). Home observations and teacher reports have been discussed as possibly better outcome measures because they are less biased and based on performance demands than parent reports of behavior (Webster- Stratton 8. Hammond, 1990). Thus, research has yet to demonstrate how several predictors may function together, how predictors function independently and how to operationally define treatment success (Wester-Stratton 8. Hammond, 1990). Consequently, it makes sense to conclude that different predictors would affect different outcomes (Wester-Stratton & Hammond, 1990). Using statistical analysis, actual variance among predictors of outcomes in parent training has been reported to be small (Webster-Stratton 8. Hammond, 1990). The data suggest that the predictor variables which were studied (including maternal depression, marital situations, SES, life stressors) may have limited utility in practice. Secondly, the authors suggested that researchers may need to look for other predictors that determine outcomes in parent training. 56 Kazdin, Holland, and Crowley (1997) label traditionally studied predictors as “variables of convenience”. Researchers may select variables to study based on easy access, such as clinic intake forms. Also, the predictors are usually demographic data which provides little information regarding mechanisms involved in outcomes nor guidelines on how to intervene in parent training to prevent negative outcomes and provide better treatments (Kazdin et al., 1997). This creates the propensity towards exclusionary practices. The solution for many PMT programs has been to carefully select clients and exclude parents who have mental health problems (Hutchings, 1996). Another example involves SES. Knowing that low SES is a predictor of negative outcomes in parent training does not provide guidelines for treatment. Kazdin et al. (1997) proposed, studied, and found support for their hypothesis regarding a conceptual model which would be predictive of parent training outcomes. The “barriers-to-treatment model” includes predictors which describe practical obstacles to participation, perceptions of treatment, and relationship with the therapist (Kazdin et al., 1997). One notable finding was that the predictor findings related to SES may be due to several mediating variables such as cost factors for transportation, more severe child-rearing strategies, more child and/or parent health problems, and discrepant parent - therapist expectations (Kazdin et al., 1997). In a meta-analysis of psychotherapy dropout, Wierzbicki and Pekarik (1993) found that psychotherapy dropout was significantly related to minority status, racial status, low education, and low SES; however, mean effect sizes 57 were moderate, ranging from .23 to .37. In fact, they concluded that these mean effect sizes are likely upper estimates of the true value of the effects, due to limitations in their study (missing data) (Wierzbicki & Pekarik, 1993). Although the study did not specifically address or examine why SES is related to dropout, the authors speculated that SES may be related to dropout because of several underlying factors such as differences among therapists and clients regarding education, value systems, and expectations (Wierzbicki 8. Pekarik, 1993). These results suggest that outcome research should focus on more complex psychological variables rather than demographic characteristics (Kazdin et al., 1997; Wierzbicki 8. Pekarik, 1993). Furthermore, parent management training can encompass many variations in treatment within the actual program and in combination with other treatments (Kazdin, 1997). This would have implications for outcome studies that attempt to identify predictors of outcome. One area of study which has not been sufficiently explored is who profits from single modality treatments such as PMT or from combined treatments (Kazdin, 1997). Also, unexplored is whether certain parents profit from certain types of PMT treatments as opposed to others. It has been hypothesized that certain parents and children may be a “good fit” for a particular type of parent training program (Holden et al., 1990). This proposal is designed in an attempt to build on previous findings regarding the predictors of PMT, identify those characteristics/ variables in advance to anticipate and provide the most effective training and prevent parents from dropping out prematurely or progressing slowly. 58 Parent Cognitions As Predictors Attention to parents’ cognitions arose within psychology as a reaction against the exclusively behavioral accounts of parenting (Goodnow, 1995). Interest in belief and attitude theories arose partially out of a reaction to simplistic behavioral explanations for the development of childhood disorders (Goodnow, 1995). Furthermore, interest in the literature on parental beliefs and attitudes is consistent with Eifert’s (1996) recommendation to use more theory driven approaches in behavior therapy complemented with individualized and manualized treatment. Yet, more general parent cognitions, such as childrearing beliefs and attitudes have been shown to be limited in predicting parent behavior (Geller & Johnston, 1995). Part of the reason may be the difficulties with defining constructs. In examining the research literature on beliefs in general, research studies demonstrate no consensus regarding what beliefs and attitudes really are and considerable confusion about the constructs. The constructs of beliefs, attitudes, perceptions, ideas, attributions, values, expectations, knowledge, cognition, perceptions, judgements, and conceptions have been used interchangeably throughout the literature (Holden, 1995; McGillicuddy-De Lisi 8 Sigel, 1995). Beliefs have been defined as knowledge or ideas that are accepted as true; attitudes defined as an evaluative aspect that is an integral component of the parent’s cognitive orientation as in “positive” or “negative"; values refer to longstanding goals that a parent holds for a child, rather than truths; both attitudes and values may include a cognitive component but these are not seen 59 as facts or truth (McGillicuddy-De Lisi 8 Sigel, 1995, p.334). Kohn (1969) in his seminal study on social class and values defined values as: the values that parents would most like to see embodied in their children’s behavior - the characteristics they consider most desirable to inculcate in their children. (Kohn, 1969, p.18). Basically, there is no generally acceptable term for belief nor any agreed upon definitions for the different cognitive constructs in the literature (McGillicuddy-De Lisi 8 Sigel, 1995). Consequently, research has shifted to other areas of parent cognitions. According to four models, (attribution, information processing, constructivist, and transactional) parental beliefs are sources of influence on the developing child, on the parents, and on the parent-child relationship (McGillicuddy-De Lisi 8 Sigel, 1995). An implicit and explicit assumption is that parental beliefs guide parental actions with children and consequently will influence childhood behavior. Some studies attempt to target the nature of parent’s beliefs and link them to childhood outcomes (McGillicuddy-De Lisi 8 Sigel, 1995). Interestingly, these studies view the age of the child as less important and do not include the children in their studies. More important is the issue of measuring these attitudes and beliefs. Studies have developed child rearing attitude surveys for predominantly descriptive, theoretical, and methodological purposes (Holden, 1995). Few studies developed surveys to serve as clinical tools. Frequently studied parental attitudes toward childrearing include four main categories: 1) attitudes related to discipline and control (authoritarian, permissive, restrictiveness, physical 60 punishment, democratic versus autocratic); 2) attitudes related to affection (acceptance, hostility, rejection, approval, warmth); 3) other parental attitudes (consistency of parenting, encouragement of independence or dependence, toilet training, role in education/teaching, over-involvement, and worries about childrearing); and, 4) attitudes related to views about child or child characteristics (assertiveness of child, conformity/deviance of child, fears of child, judgements about child behaviors, sex behavior of child, achievement of child, approval of child, emotional health of child, health of child, sex role expectations of child, and verbal output of child) (Holden, 1995). There were few psychometrically solid, formal instruments available for measuring parent cognitions (Johnston, 1996). Earlier, seminal research utilized parent interviews to measure parent cognitions. Baumrind (1971) and Baumrind and Black (1967) categorized parental attitudes into authoritarian, authoritative, and permissive types based in part, upon interviews. Attitude surveys sometimes include descriptive beliefs, instrumental beliefs, behavioral intentions, and values. Parents have descriptive beliefs about childrearing which are beliefs about how children develop or how parents affect children (Holden, 1995). For example, the statement, most children are toilet trained by 15 months is a descriptive belief. Parents also have instrumental beliefs about childrearing which are defined as the ways in which parents can achieve particular goals (Holden, 1995). For example, the statement, Ibelieve physical punishment is the best way to discipline, is an instrumental belief. It is 61 these instrumental beliefs which are of the most interest and may have the most applicability to outcomes in parent management therapies. Behavioral intentions are simply the intention to behave in a particular way (Holden, 1995). Few childrearing surveys explicitly address childrearing practices which focus on specific behaviors and how often parents behave in that manner. The surveys that do, include questions such as how often do you require your child to put away his or her clothes” (Holden, 1995). Values, perceptions, and self-perceptions are also addressed in some childrearing surveys. Values are regarded as superordinate categories that reflect abstract goals or a coherent set of attitudes (Holden, 1995). An example of the former is to raise a happy child while an example of the latter is respect one’s parents. Perceptions address parents’ views or reactions to their child (Holden, 1995) . For example, items assess views about how difficult the child is (Holden, 1995). Self-perceptions refer to parents’ attitudes about their own abilities and their relationship with their child (Holden, 1995). For example, an item here might include the statement, I find the greatest satisfaction in my child. Research has demonstrated over and over again, that parents’ childrearing attitudes and beliefs explain only a small proportion of the variance in parenting behavior (Geller 8 Johnston, 1995). This suggests that attitudes and beliefs are not the sole determinants of parenting behavior and that other cognitions may play a role (Geller 8 Johnston, 1995). First several investigators have examined the interaction among parents’ beliefs and attitudes with single 62 parenthood, SES, parent psychopathology, and life stressors. Second, investigators have begun to examine other cognitions. Interactions Among Teps And Parent Cognitions Studies have examined the relationship among childrearing attitudes and several traditional examined predictors (marital status and maternal insularity, SES, maternal depression, and life stressors) as determinants in parenting behavior. Marital status and adjustment has also been linked to various childrearing attitudes. Goldberg (1990) replicated previous work and again found that mothers in happy marriages reported attitudes of greater warmth and more encouragement of independence in their children while mothers in unhappy marriages reported more aggravation with their children. Cox et al. ( 1989) in a study of 38 couples with infants, reported that mothers in close, confiding marriages were warmer and more sensitive and fathers in the same marriages had more positive childrearing attitudes than parents in less happy marriages. Kohn (1969) hypothesized about the relationship between attitudes and social class. Kohn found substantial support for his theory which relates social class to values to parent behavior. It is noteworthy that social class was evaluated by the Hollingshead Index which is based on education and occupation. This index leaves out income and subjective class identification. He proposed that working class parents value conformity to external rules, emphasize obedience and good manners, neatness, and impose constraints on their children. Furthermore, these values are the result of life conditions, i.e., pa rent job requirements associated with working under supervision, and following 63 instructions. In contrast, parents from higher occupational levels value self direction, self-control, curiosity, creativity, and are more supportive of independence in their children in keeping with their job requirements. Early studies have linked maternal depression to parent attitudes. Scores on the Parent Attitude Research Instrument (PARI), specifically, hostile childrearing attitude scores were associated with anxiety and depression in mothers (Holden, 1995). Brunnquell, Chrichton, and Egeland (1981) in a large study of 267 high risk mothers described the interaction of childrearing attitudes and personality variables as the best predictors of quality of care infants received with implications for exploring intervention in mother-child disturbed relationships. Neither single attitudes nor single personality variables were discriminant of high functioning versus lower functioning mothers. Hall et al. (1991) in a study of 225 mothers found low income, single mothers to be at high risk for depressive symptoms. Maternal depression symptoms predicted parent attitudes which in turn predicted child behavior (Hall et al., 1991). Research also supports the findings that depression promotes negative perceptions of child behavior and is associated with more negative attributions for child behavior (Johnston, 1996). Maternal insularity and social support has also been linked to various childrearing attitudes. Cochran and Niegro (1995) found the presence or absence of social supports impacted on maternal attitudes and adjustment. Crnic and Acevedo (1995) found that social support and stress predicted maternal attitudes toward satisfaction in parenting. Crockenberg (1987) in a study of 40 mothers with two year old children found that mothers who experienced little support and 64 were, themselves rejected by their parents, held negative, punitive, and angry attitudes towards childrearing. In an extension of the research associating life stressors with parent perceptions of child deviant behavior, (Johnston, 1996) reports that major life stressors differentially affect mothers’ perceptions of child behavior. Thus, links have been drawn among other traditional predictors (maternal depression and marital adjustment) and parent attitudes. Potential Motivational Predictors Of Outcomes In Pmt Many of the problems parents face in managing the disruptive and inappropriate behavior of their children are likely motivational in nature. Considerable research has been done in the general area of motivation in parenting behavior and child development outcomes (Goodnow, 1995; Holden, 1995; Lisi 8 Sigel, 1995). One basic premise has been that parent child rearing “attitudes” and their core philosophies guide their behavior and ultimately, the child’s development (Gillis-Amold, Crase, Stockdale, 8 Shelley, 1998). The present proposal contends that many of the problems and negative outcomes in PMT programs also contain a motivational in nature mediating component. Furthermore, these motivational mediating components are central to parent’s inner processes and not only the child’s inner processes (which is an entire other proposal). A definition of motivation will help illustrate these points: Motivation is the study of the internal processes that give behavior its energy and direction......motivation originates from a variety of sources (needs, cognitions, and emotions), and that these internal processes energize and direct behavior in multiple ways such as starting, sustaining, intensifying, focusing, and stopping it. (Reeve, 1996, p. 2). 65 It has long been established that student motivation has a direct causal relation to student achievement (Brophy, 1998). Parents involved in PMT programs are essentially involved in achievement type situations — hence the term, parent education with some programs. Consequently, it stands to reason that parental motivational factors influence parent “achievement” in parent management training programs, and ultimately, translate into goal directed behavior which facilitates either positive or negative outcomes of PMT. Reeve (1996) describes eleven inner motivational resources including psychological needs (self-determination, competence, and relatedness), cognitive domains (self-efficacy, personal control beliefs, achievement strivings, goal setting, and the self), and emotional states (curiosity, interest, and enjoyment/positive effect). Brophy (1998) defines motivation as “a theoretical construct used to explain the initiation, direction, intensity, and persistence of behavior, especially goal directed behavior” (Brophy, 1998, p. 3). Organizing much of the motivational research within an expectancy x value model, he explains that, the effort that people are willing to expend on a task is the product of 1) the degree to which they expect to be able to perform the task successfully if they apply themselves (and thus the degree to which they expect to get whatever rewards that successful task performance will bring and 2) the degree to which they value those rewards as well as the opportunity to engage in the processes involved in performing the task itself. (Brophy, 1998, p.14). Few of these inner motivational resources and value aspects have been directly and explicitly examined within the context of parent management training (PMT) outcome research. Only a handful of studies have alluded to the influence 66 of parent motivation within the context of PMT outcomes and mostly in a peripheral fashion (Table 10). One study which explicitly examined motivation within a PMT program, taught parents “motivational” strategies for changing child behavior and did not address the motivational constructs underlying the strategies (Koegel, Bimbela, 8 Schreibman, 1996). Another study examined motivations for foster parenting in a foster parent training program (Gillis-Arnold et al., 1998). Other studies did not mention the word motivation or related motivational aspects, although the content suggested elements consistent with the motivational constructs of goals (First 8 Way, 1995) and value aspects of motivation (Kazdin, Holland, 8 Crowley, 1997; Kazdin 8 Wassell, 1999). A few studies directly addressed the motivational aspects of self-efficacy but not as predictors of PMT outcomes but as a parent outcome of participation in PMT (Thompson, Ruma, Schuchmann, 8 Burke, 1996; (Tucker et al., 1998); and, as a pre-post measure of self-efficacy regarding the use of children’s mental health services (Bickman et al., 1998; Heflinger et al., 1997). Outside the context of PMT but within a situation responsive nursing program, one study examined parent’s general self-efficacy and locus of control as related to health promotion behavior (Ford-Gilboe, 1997). Those resources pertaining to parent’s cognitive domains (self efficacy theory, personal control beliefs/attributions, achievement strivings/goal theory, self theory, and value) are the focus of the present proposal and postulated to be predictive of outcomes in PMT. Early seminal studies are credited for their theories of motivation and implications for understanding thought and behavior 67 000000 .00.>0000 0:00 0. 00.0.0. 3009.0 .00.: 000.00: .0 00:00.0. 00200.00 .00.00 .0500. 00.08.00 :0 0.00:0 00 00.0.0: >000_...0 ._00 0.00 .0.0000: >0 00.02.0E 02.000000: .200 02.0 .0. 0.00:0.000E00 .00.0..00 02.0.0 00.00.00. .0.00:0.:00:.00 0.20 00000. 0. 00.02.00. 0.00. 02.0000 0.00000 00_0> 00.00._0E. .00E.00: 0. 0.0.00: 0.... .0 000 >000...0.._00 00:00.00 .0.00. .0. 05:02.0... 0.02.0000 00 000.20... 002.098 00 00.0.0. .0 .00.:000005. .00.0n. 0...... 00.0..00 .000: .0.000>> w 0.0~0v. 300.90 0005 00”.. 00000 0:00.. .00.0.0 .00:..0.<.00Em .0.00E 000:0> C00 5 00000.0:00 0200.00 ...0.0000> 0...: 00.0..00 .0 .00.0002 0000.05 00000.0... E0_00...00 0.00.00 .0.00. 00.0.0. .. 003.000.... ”00:00.00 02.000000: 800 3 >205 .0.000 .0. 02.0000... 0.02.0. 0:0 02.0000 .0 0.000.020 .000.0 .0.00.<-0.__.0 08:00.0 >0._000 .0002. 0:000 >000....0 ._00 .0.0000 00.0.00 00......0. 0. 00..00.0:.00 2.0.0. .00..0 ._0::00 .0 02000000. 00:02.0 ".210. .00.00 02.. 0:0 >.0>:.000 00:02.9: .0 000.000 0000. 0000: .00.0.0. .0005. 0:00... .0.00E00_0>0n. .00.00 0.00.0 .0005 .s. 08.5-0.0“. 0:.E00. 0>:0:..0.000.. u 000.00.00 00.00000 ._00 .0.000 Cwn. .00. .00:00:00>0. .00.00_0000.00000 00.00..0:.. 0.00.020 .0.0>00 .0 0.00E0.0 00.00.00: .000 3 >0>> a .0..... 00.00800 0000290. .00.00 300.0... 0005 00“.. 00000 5.000 000 000.00.00 .0.>0000 0:00 :0 .00.0.0 .00E.0z.00.:w .0.00E 000..0> 800: 00....000 .0 000020000 0.00.00 0: 00.0.0: >000...0 ..00 >000_...0.._00 0200.00 ._.0.0000> 0...... 00.0..00 .00.0002 0000.00... 00.00.05 00:02.00. 0. 0....0000.000.00.0000 .00:0:00 0.9.00.0 00:0.0000 >020 05:02.05. 0:65 .0 :20 .0.x0.000 :0:00>.0.0. .M..0..m .0 .220 0.5.2. 00.00..0> .0:0:0>:0.2 00.0.E0xw 00.030 or 0.00... 68 00.00.00. 00000.0 00..00.00 0. 0000000 02..000 0. 000. .000 0000.000 .00.0.00.. .0. .0000 .000:.00.0 00.00.00 00 5000.0 .00.00 00 .0000 00 00.0.0 .0.00E 000 000 .00.00 02.0000 .000 .00.0.00.. .060. 00.0.0: 00:02.00. 0...... 000..00.0.0. .00.00-0_.00 02.000 0.00. .005 0000.000 .00.0.00.. .0. .00..0 .000:.00.0 0.0.000 .00.0 0000.0. .0050. .00 0. E0800: .00. 00:08.00 0000.00. .00E.00.. 0. 0.0.000 000.020 0.030.500.0003. 5000.0 ._00 C00. 00.0.0: .00.00 .0.0.>0000 .00000. 008.00.. 000 .000.> 00.0 0.0. 00:00:00. .00.... 008.... .0000. 5000.0 ..00 0.....0 .00.0000000. .0.>0000 00.0.2.0. 2.0.0.00 000 0.00.0..0 0.02.0. 0.00. 0x00. 0.0 0...... 2.00 x.0. .00.000 2.6 00. 0...... ”00.0.0 >00.0..0 0.0.0.0000 00 .000 m... 0.02.0... 0. 2.00 00 00.00.000. 00000000. .0820 00.0000. + 032 .N .0.>0000 .00.0. 00.0000 0. .. H00:00:.000. .0.>0000 00.0000. .020. 00.0.0... .00E00000s. .00.00 .2000 2.6 .5. C000. 0... -00.. 0:00 9.3.00 0.0.8.0 00:02.00. .0 0.00000 00_0> 00.00..00.. 0.208. .020... 02.... 0.00.090 .0.>0000 0...... 00.0....0 0.0..00 5.2. 00.2.6 .0.000..00 w .0>.000.000 ._000...00000 0...... 00.0....0 .000..0000000 a $000.00 .0000 .0005 00000.0 .000: 9.50 w .000E00000m .00.00 .0000E00... .000: 00:52.00 0 0005.0 .0008. £00.. .2390 .0 0:00: .00.00. c. 0.00... 0....00 69 related to self efficacy (Bandura, 1997), causal attributions (Weiner, 1979), and performance and Ieaming goals (Dweck & Leggett, 1988). These and some of the more recent studies in motivation regarding other domain specific areas provide the groundwork for application to PMT. Within each motivational section, a review of relevant studies examining motivational factors which influence parenting in general are provided as well as a discussion of the sparse, PMT research which can be related to these motivational aspects. The motivational factors discussed below (self efficacy, personal control beliefs, achievement strivings, goal theories, implicit theory, self theory, and value) are those which are the focus of the present study and predominantly apply to PMT outcomes. Self Efficacy Theory Self efficacy is concerned with how well or poorly people judge or believe that they will perform on a task given their skills and a variety of circumstances (Bandura, 1997, p.37). It is the individuals’ beliefs about their abilities and about the outcomes of their actions which influence the many ways in which they behave (Pajares, 1996). For example, individuals avoid tasks in which they feel incompetent and engage in tasks in which they feel competent (Pajares, 1996). Self efficacy beliefs originate from four sources of information including performance accomplishments, vicarious experience (modeling), verbal persuasion, and physiological state, in decreasing order of potency of influence (Reeve, 1996). Performance accomplishments are first hand mastery experiences in which successes increase efficacy while failures decrease efficacy beliefs (Reeve, 1996). Several caveats to these maxims are given which 70 implicate perceived difficulty of the task, the amount of physical guidance received, and the timing of the pattern of successes and failures (Reeve, 1996). The second source of information, vicarious experience, occurs when people base their efficacy beliefs on a social comparison process with others (Reeve, 1996). Furthermore, efficacy beliefs are strengthened when observers watch coping models (who struggle and cope to overcome difficult tasks) as opposed to mastery models (who expertly perform flawlessly) and when they watch multiple models (Reeve, 1996). The third source of information upon which individuals base efficacy beliefs, verbal persuasion, occurs when others (teacher, parent, coach, peer, employer, therapist, audience, inspirational poster, song) give a successful pep talk (Reeve, 1996). The persuader must be credible, trustworthy, and have some “expertise” in order for verbal persuasion to have an influence on efficacy expectations (Reeve, 1996). The least potent source informing efficacy beliefs, physiological state, comes from experiences prior to, or during task performances (Reeve, 1996). Autonomic nervous system arousal (increased heart rate, respiratory rate, trembling hands), somatic arousal (muscle tension, fatigue, pains and aches), and cortical arousal (rate of thinking, mental confusion) sometimes occur during some performances (Reeve, 1996). The degree to which they are perceived to be “under control” heightens efficacy beliefs (Reeve, 1996). Self efficacy beliefs have been found to have diverse influence on student’s academic functioning including choice of activities and selection of 71 environments; extent of effort and persistence put forth during task performance; quality of thinking and decision making during performance; and, emotional reactions, related to stress and anxiety (Reeve, 1996). Students with low self efficacy likely “shun and avoid activities and situations they see as likely to overwhelm their coping capacities” and arrest developmental potentials (Bandura, 1997; Reeve, 1996). Similarly, parents with low self efficacy may be likely to drop out of PMT. Students with strong self efficacy beliefs choose active engagements that contribute to growth of competencies (Reeve, 1996). Students with low self efficacy slacken their efforts or give up when things get difficult. Students with high self efficacy persist with coping efforts when things get difficult (Reeve, 1996). Again, parents with high self efficacy are more likely to follow through with PMT suggestions and to complete homework assignments. Students with low efficacy become cognitively erratic when problem solving is required (Reeve, 1996). Students with high efficacy become highly efficient in their analytic thinking during complex decision making (Reeve, 1996). This latter finding is among the most salient in reference to parent efficacy when judging how well or effectively, they can manage their child’s disruptive behavior. In order to maximize the efficacy of PMT involvement, parents must use memories of past events to generate hypotheses about the most effective course of action, analyze feedback to assess the validity of their plans and strategies, reflect upon performances and remember which were most effective and which not. Parents who are low in self efficacy are likely high in self doubt which deteriorates and impairs the thinking and decision making process, especially in the face of 72 situational stress and problem solving failures (Reeve, 1996). Finally, students high in self efficacy, visualize success scenarios and react to challenges with enthusiastic effort, optimism, and interest (Reeve, 1996). Students low in self efficacy dwell on personal deficiencies, visualize formidable obstacles, and react to challenges with pessimism, anxiety, and depression (Reeve, 1996). Basically, events offer potentially aversive experiences and consequences but individuals offer coping capabilities which can counteract the potentially ovenlvhelming aspects of the event (Reeve, 1996, p. 89). The above discussion is central to parent self efficacy beliefs regarding their own capabilities. Also relevant here, is the teacher efficacy literature which involves “an instructor’s belief or conviction that he or she can influence how well students learn, especially students considered to be difficult or amotivated” (Reeve, 1996, p. 89). Because teacher efficacy influences a teacher’s selection of activities, effort, and persistence (Reeve, 1996), it is also likely that parent efficacy influences the same. Parents with high efficacy may also believe that they can bring about “Ieaming, performance, and achievement” in their children. Parents with low efficacy avoid offering activities that have the potential to be unmanageable, give up quickly, and forgo the effort to reteach or try again. Although measuring teacher efficacy is difficult, three types of items are deemed to represent the essence of teacher self efficacy (Reeve, 1996): 1. When I really try, I can get through to most difficult students. 2. If a student did not remember information I gave in a previous lesson, I would know how to increase his or her retention in the next lesson. 3. If a student in my class becomes disruptive and noisy, I feel assured that I know some techniques to redirect him quickly. (Reeve, 1996, p. 90) 73 Similar statements from surveys assessing self efficacy and supported through factor analysis procedures include the following five items: 1. PWN If I really try hard, I can get through to even the most difficult or unmotivated student. If some students in my class are not doing well in math, I feel that I should change my approach to the subject. By trying a different teaching method, I can significantly affect a student’s achievement. There is very little I can do to insure that most of my students achieve at a high level. I am certain I am making a difference in the lives of my students. (Midgley, Feldlaufer, & Eccles, 1989) By altering the domain specificity of these items, these same types of items can be translated and applied to parent self efficacy: 1. 2. 4. 5. When I really try, I can get through to my son or daughter when he or she is being most difficult. If my son or daughter did not remember the last time we had a “scene” about a certain house rule, I would know how to increase his or her memory the next time. If my child became disruptive and noisy in public places (malls, church), I feel assured that I know some techniques to redirect him or her quickly. By trying a different discipline method, I can significantly affect my child’s behavior. There is really very little I can do insure that my child behaves appropriately. (With one additional related question), 6. If I could choose to have children, if I had the decision to make over again, I would choose parenthood. Notably, these statements involve judgements which are task and domain specific. In order to be clear about what is being measured and to differentiate other expectancy beliefs from self efficacy beliefs, assessment requires context- specific judgments at a micro-analytic level (Pajares, 1996). From the 74 perspective of more task and situation specific judgements, the following statements may also apply to parent efficacy: 1. I am confident that I can make the decisions that are made in our home (parent efficacy). 2. I am confident that I can have my son or daughter listen to me and comply when I am discussing household chores (self-efficacy for engaging compliant behavior). 3. I am confident that I can learn the material with the other parents in these sessions (collective efficacy). (adapted from Pajares, 1996) Collective efficacy is defined as “a group’s shared belief in their capabilities to realize given levels of attainment” (Bandura, 1997). Furthermore, this collective efficacy has been reported to mediate the influence of students’ socioeconomic status and prior academic achievement (Bandura, 1997). Coupled with the findings from a number of studies which have demonstrated the relationship between teacher efficacy and student achievement (Midgley et al., 1989), parent efficacy beliefs are likely to have similar influences on achievement in parent training and ultimately child behavior. It is also possible that the group parent training “collective efficacy” may serve to mediate the effects of traditionally examined predictors, such as SES, in PMT outcomes. A series of studies on The Family Empowerment Project is among the sparse literature found which remotely examines the relationship between parent self efficacy and PMT (Bickman et al., 1998; Heflinger et al., 1997). Bickman et al. (1998) utilized a multi-component parent training curriculum to enhance parent empowerment and increase their involvement in mental service delivery for their children with special needs. While this training does not meet the 75 definition of PMT nor apply exclusively to children with disruptive behavior disorders, the program included mental health service’s self efficacy as a key aspect and novel addition (Heflinger et al., 1997). The four self efficacy sources of information (performance accomplishments, vicarious experience/modelling, verbal persuasion, and physiological state) were used as training techniques to help parents perceive themselves as capable of acquiring and using newly acquired knowledge and skills about the mental health system to improve their children’s treatment. The training of mental health services self efficacy specifically addressed (a) encouraging active participation in decision making, (b) promoting collaborative relationships between parents and professionals, (c) modeling participation in a support group (Heflinger et al., 1997). This series of evaluations indicated that the training intervention increased parent mental health services efficacy but parent knowledge and self efficacy was not related to parent involvement, service use, or children’s mental health (Bickman et al., 1998; Heflinger et al., 1997). This suggests that parents’ confidence in their knowledge of service systems and skills needed to interact with service systems is not related to better outcomes, in this case as measured by increased involvement. However, these studies still leave open the question as to whether parents’ confidence in their knowledge of child management and skills needed to apply that knowledge, promotes better outcomes in PMT. Two other studies examined self-efficacy within the context of PMT but explored self-efficacy as an outcome measure of participation in the PMT interventions (Thompson et al., 1996; Tucker et al., 1998). Thompson et al. 76 (1996) examined cost effective PMT treatments and evaluated changes in parent self-efficacy as an outcome of participation. Specifically, the authors found that a modified, less time consuming and less costly PMT program is an effective treatment on post test outcome measures including child behavior changes, parent attitudes and satisfaction with family relationships. As noted previously, the authors also found that marital status did not predict outcomes in parenting efficacy or parenting satisfaction (Thompson et al., 1996). However, outside the context of PMT, Johnston (1996), discusses the reciprocal nature of disruptive child behaviors and parenting efficacy. This may suggest a link between self- efficacy and severity of child behavior, another TEP. Tucker and colleagues, (1998) examined the effectiveness of a behavioral parent training (BPT) intervention which is another name for PMT. The authors found that positive changes in parenting self efficacy, parenting stress, and parent-toddler interactions resulted from participation in a PMT intervention. A study peripheral to PMT examined general self efficacy and locus of control, noted below in the attribution section (Ford-Gilboe, 1997). The term general self efficacy was used to denote a general belief in one’s capabilities to successfully perform behaviors across situations (Ford-Gilboe, 1997). They found a moderate correlation between general self-efficacy and health behavior. The authors point out that the usefulness of general self-efficacy as a predictor of specific health behaviors has been debated (Ford-Gilboe, 1997). Given the lack of clarity and domain specificity of efficacy beliefs, it is possible that other motivational expectancy aspects were measured as well. 77 Personal Control Beliefs Personal control beliefs relate to the level of mastery an individual feels over life’s outcomes and involve the two psychological constructs of causal attributions and explanatory style (Reeve, 1996). A causal attribution is an explanation of why a particular outcome comes to pass and most often involves the following five attributions: ability, effort, task difficulty, strategy, and luck (Reeve, 1996). Because any list of causal attributions is possible, attributions are organized along three dimensions: 1) internal versus external; 2) stable versus unstable; 3) controllable versus uncontrollable (Reeve, 1996). Internal versus external attributions involve those that exist within the individual (intelligence) versus those that exist outside, in the environment (help from others), respectively (Reeve, 1996). Stable versus unstable attributes involve those that are permanent (personality traits) versus those that are temporary (moods), respectively (Reeve, 1996). Controllable versus uncontrollable attributions involve those that the performer can take responsibility (personal effort) versus those that the performer cannot (luck), respectively (Reeve, 1996). The three attributional dimensions can be grouped into two explanatory styles (pessimistic or optimistic) which are cognitive personality variables that reflect the habitual manner in which people explain the cause of bad events that befall them (Reeve, 1996, p. 98). Table 11 illustrates the explanatory styles and related attributional dimensions within the context of child misbehavior (any behavior or task 78 .8? exam sec 8382 .862 293.0550 o_nm__obcooc3 23:05:08: .m> 223.9280 295mg: 03ma mimic: .m> ofifim .9:ka .mEQE .manxm. .m> .mEEE o=m_E_EO o=m_E_mmon_ 229885 .330 23w boamcmaxw ._o_>mcmnm_s_ EEO GEE—30m mcoficmEE .mcozantzd. 3.5. m5 c_£_>> nmNtodmumo 23¢, anmcflaxw E. 2an 79 can be substituted here). Hypothetically speaking, parents with optimistic explanatory styles, attribute their child’s misbehavior to 1) external, 2) unstable, 3) controllable dimensions and endorse related statements as follows: 1. When my child is in one of his/her moods, there is usually something that set him off but can be handled. 2. My child gets oppositional, hostile, or defiant because of situational circumstances, he can be appeased with appropriate discipline. 3. My child experiences difficulties because of his school or home environments which can be altered to suit his/her needs. In contrast, parents with pessimistic explanatory styles attribute their child’s misbehavior to 1) internal, 2) stable, 3) uncontrollable dimensions and endorse related statements as follows: 1. My child’s behavior is mostly biological, a big part of his/her personality, and sometimes, we’re lucky to get through one of his/her temper tantrums. 2. There is little influential relationship between my behavior and how my child behaves, especially since s/he has a hostile and defiant temperament. 3. There is little to be gained from an open, accepting, and supportive relationship with my child, especially since my child is unmanageable. It is this latter, pessimistic explanatory style which predisposes students to a passive approach to studying and Ieaming and also leads to a helpless motivational orientation whereas students with an optimistic explanatory style develop a mastery motivational orientation and persist in the face of difficulties (Reeve, 1996). Thus, parents with a pessimistic attributional style may also be predisposed to similar approaches to learning and consequently, negative outcomes in parent training. 80 Three recent studies discuss the association between parent attributions and parenting behavior outside the context of PMT. Ford- Gilboe (1997) examined the impact of motivational aspects (and other resources) on health promotion among single versus two parent families. Locus of control, an attributional motivational aspect, defined as the family’s beliefs about their ability to influence health, was one of the sources. A second source of motivation was general efficacy defined as a general belief in one’s capability to successfully perform behaviors across situations. Both, internal health locus of control and general self efficacy were positively correlated with participation in health work. A second study examined mothers’ attributions and general parenting attitudes and their relation to responses to child noncompliance (Geller & Johnston, 1995). These authors’ review of the literature consistently linked parent attributions of child behavior with parenting responses (Geller 8 Johnston, 1995). Similarly, the study findings also indicated that mothers’ attributions for both child and parent behavior were predictive of particular parental responses. In particular, maternal depressed mood was also associated with more negative attributions for child behavior, suggesting a link between a well established TEP and a more pessimistic explanatory style. Third, in a basic mediational model of parenting children with disruptive behaviors, Johnston (1996) included the following four parent cognitions: a) expectations for child behavior; b) perceptions of child behavior; c) attributions for 81 child behavior; and, d) sense of parenting efficacy. Among the four parent cognitions discussed, the latter two were obviously motivational in nature. Interestingly, lines of research have examined parent attributions regarding child intentional (malicious defiance) and unintentional (fidgety, restlessness) child behaviors, characteristics of CD and ADHD, respectively (Johnston, 1996). Young adult raters were better able to discriminate between these behaviors when presented separately than when they co-occurred (Johnston, 1996). Thus, the raters were found to have difficulty matching their attributions to the intentionality of the child’s behavior, blaming children for unintentional fidgeting while, conversely, excusing children for overt defiance (Johnston, 1996). Given the prevalence for comorbidity among the disruptive behavior disorders, this has implications for parent attributions and parenting behavior. If we consider that parent cognitions predict the outcome of parent-child interactions, these studies support the notion that parent attributions may play a role in PMT outcomes. Achievement Stn'vings Students’ adaptive or maladaptive patterns of motivation influence whether students seek or avoid academic challenges, exert or withhold effort, persist or give up in the face of failure, and work to develop skills efficiently (Reeve, 1996). Dweck termed the adaptive motivational pattern as a mastery orientation and a maladaptive pattern as a learned helpless orientation (Reeve, 1996). These patterns are not necessarily related to ability or intelligence but to two types of goals which students adopt in 82 achievement situations (Reeve, 1996). The two types of goals pertaining to the achievement/competence class of goals (Dweck, 1996) are defined as follows: 1) Learning goals: students approach academic challenges with the desire to increase their competence, gain task mastery, improve knowledge and skill, and understand something new. This orientation leads students to ask the question, What is the best way to increase my ability or achieve mastery? 2) Performance goals: students approach academic challenges with the desire to gain favorable and avoid unfavorable evaluations of their competence and to measure/evaluate their ability. This orientation leads students to ask the question, Is my ability adequate or inadequate? (Dweck & Leggett, 1988; Reeve, 1996) Furthermore, perceptions of ability combine with these two goal orientations to produce different levels of achievement behavior (Dweck, 1996; Reeve, 1996). Specifically, students with high performance goals and high ability perception sacrifice learning and choose moderate or moderately difficult tasks to display competence. In response to difficulty, these students demonstrate a mastery orientation of effective problem solving. Students with performance goals and low ability perception sacrifice learning and choose moderately easy tasks to avoid displays of non-competence. In response to difficulty these students demonstrate a leamed-helpless response, low quality problem solving strategies, impaired performance, lowered persistence, and negative affect (Dweck, 1996; Elliot & Dweck, 1988). In contrast, students with Ieaming goals and either high or low ability choose Ieaming at the risk of displaying mistakes to increase competence, focus on effort or strategy. maintain positive 83 affect, and demonstrate a mastery orientation of effective problem solving in response to difficulty or failure (Dweck, 1996; Elliot & Dweck, 1988). Parents who demonstrate a mastery orientation with Ieaming goals may endorse statements such as the following: Making mistakes is a part of Ieaming. I work hard to learn. The therapist makes sure I understand the work. The therapist wants us to try new things. (adapted from Ames 8 Archer, 1988) PP’NT“ Altemately, parents who demonstrate a learned helpless orientation with performance goals may endorse statements such as the following: Other parents want to know how I am doing on assignments. I really don’t like to make mistakes. Only a few parents will get a lot out of this parent training. I work hard to get the homework assignments done. Parents feel bad when they do not do as well as others. (adapted from Ames & Archer, 1988) WPWN.‘ Parents with exceptionally high scores on performance goals may be more likely to be obsessed with self-worth protection and public appearances than with Ieaming goals, i.e., on how to make the situation better. This is somewhat related to the notion that overall classroom setting, and possibly school and even community culture may play a role in students’ goal orientations (Lemos, 1996). Goal Theories No published research has been found, to date, which examines the role that PMT interventions play in parents’ goal orientations. Dweck (1996) delineates three classes of goals within goal theory frameworks and patterns. The first class of goals (previously mentioned 84 above) focus on the goals that people adopt in achievement/competence situations and can be categorized according to Ieaming goals, performance goals, and work avoidant goals (Brophy, 1998). Learning goals are adopted when students focus on trying to learn whatever they need to know in order to develop the skills necessary for the task (Brophy, 1998). Synonymous terms in the literature include learning goals (Dweck 8 Leggett, 1988) and mastery goals (Ames 8 Archer, 1988). Performance goals are adopted when students focus more on themselves than on the task, i.e., on their self-perceptions, and on their public reputations (Brophy, 1998). Synonymous terms include performance goals (Ames 8 Archer, 1988; Dweck 8 Leggett, 1988) and ego-involvement or ego-oriented goals (Midgley, Arunkumar, 8 Urdan, 1996; Nicholls, 1984). Work avoidant goals are adopted when students refuse to accept the academic challenge and minimize time and effort to it (Meece 8 Holt, 1998). The Patterns of Adaptive Learning Survey (PALS) has been factor analyzed and provides evidence of construct validity and contains items measuring an ego-oriented goal orientation (performance goals) and a learning-oriented goal orientation (Midgley et al., 1996). A recent study (Midgley et al., 1996) examined self-handicapping orientations and adapted the following five Ieaming oriented items: 1. I feel most successful in school when I learn something I didn’t know before. . The main reason I do my work is because I like to learn. 2 3. I like school work the best when it really makes me think. 4. I like school work I’ll learn from even if I make a lot of mistakes. 85 5. Understanding the work in school is more important to me than the grades I get. As well as the following four ego-oriented goals: 1. 2. 3. 4. I would feel successful in school if I did better than the other students in my class. I like school work that lets me show how smart I am. I’d like to show my teachers that I’m smarter than the other kids. I’d like to show my parents that I’m smarter than the other kids in my class. Translating, adapting these items to similar items relating to PMT, the following items may suggest similar constructs: Learning Oriented Goals: 1. 2. 5. I will feel most successful in parent training when I learn something I didn’t know before. The main reason I am doing my parent training homework is because I like to learn. 3. I like parent training work best when it makes me think. 4. I like parent training work I'll Ieam from even if I make a lot of mistakes. Understanding the parent training work is more important to me than getting evaluated (others looking at my behavior and approving). Performance Goals: 1. 2. 3. 4. I would feel successful in parent training if I did better than the other parents in the sessions. I would like parent training activities that let me show how good I am at managing behavior. I would like to show my group leader, therapist, that I am better at managing behavior than the other parents. I would like to show my other relatives who are parents (my parents, siblings, cousins), that I am better at managing behavior than other parents. Two possible dimensions of performance (ego-oriented goals) have been identified (self-enhancing and self-defeating ego orientations) and analyzed separately (Skaalvik, 1997). The above examples of performance goal statements (Ames, 1988; Midgely et al., 1996) define goals in terms of ego-enhancing ego orientations. Students with strong 86 self-enhancing ego orientations adapt performance goals which strive to demonstrate superior abilities, do better than others, or outperform others (Skaalvik, 1997). Students with strong self-defeating orientations adapt performance goals which strive to avoid looking stupid and being negatively judged by others (Skaalvik, 1997). Factor analysis studies identifies the following statements as related to self-defeating ego orientations: 1. :‘f-SON N991 When I answer questions in class, I am occupied by how I am perceived by other students. When lam working on the blackboard I am concerned about what my classmates think about me. At school lam concemed not to make a fool of myself. When I give a wrong answer in class lam most concemed about what my classmates think about me. The worst thing about doing mistakes at school is that other students may notice. At school, it is important for me to avoid looking stupid. At school, I try not to be among the poorest students. (Skaalvik, 1997) Applied to PMT and parents, the above statements are reformulated: 1. 2. 3 Whenever I talk in group sessions, lam occupied (I wonder) by how I am perceived by other parents. When [participate in the group activities, I am concerned about what my peers think about me. . When interacting with my child in public places, I am concerned not to make a fool of myself. Whenever I talk about how unsuccessful homework assignments have been at home, I am most concerned about what other parents think about me. The worst thing about making mistakes at home and discussing them in groups is what other people may notice. At home, it is important for me to avoid looking stupid. At group sessions, I try to avoid being among the poorest students/parents. 87 Students, preoccupied with social comparisons and highly concerned with not being perceived as stupid, are particularly vulnerable in achievement situations (Skaalvik, 1997). Thus, parents, similarly preoccupied, may be predisposed to negative outcomes in PMT programs. Dweck's (1996) second class of goals, interpersonal or relationship goals, contain two subclasses of goals which are analogous to learning and performance goals. Developmental goals involve a focus on building relationships, seeking intimacy, helping others, and seeking to improve one’s relationship skills and are more likely to produce mastery-oriented responses (focus on attributions to controllable factors, maintenance of positive affect, and increased self-disclosure) in the face of failure or rejection (Dweck, 1996). Approval goals involve a focus on striving for validation, seeking approval, acceptance, and are more likely to produce helpless responses in the face of failure or rejection (Dweck, 1996). Dweck's (1996) third and fourth classes of goals are viewed as social dominance and control goals and as hedonic goals (focus on pleasure or pain or on concrete rewards or punishment), respectively. When the focus is on dominating, controlling, or seeking power over others, control goals have been shown to be predictive of hostile attributions and aggression (Dweck, 1996). All goals are described as universal in that any individual is likely to pursue all classes at various times (Dweck, 1996). However, different classes have been shown to 88 create different vulnerabilities when certain goals predominate (Dweck, 1996) Other theorists have reformulated and discussed other types of goals, other than strictly achievement oriented have been described in Ieaming situations. For example, students may adopt up to seven highly distinct types of goals in order of descending priorities: academic orientations (working, evaluation, and learning goals), social and interpersonal orientations (complying, discipline, and interpersonal relationship goals), and enjoyment orientations (enjoyment goals) (Lemos, 1996). Moreover, these student goals appear to be consistent in that they bring to the classroom setting a general goal orientation (e.g. working) and implement specific goals when faced with the real setting (Lemos, 1996). Performance goals were apparent in the phrases used by parents describing their experiences and outcomes of participating in parent education classes (First 8 Way, 1995). Although this phenomenological study did not discuss parent responses within the context of goal theory, parent responses interpreted as belonging to the category of happier outcomes, more confidence, and feeling like “good” parents contained elements of self enhancing performance goals. For instance, parents made statements such as “People say. . .here’s somebody we can look up to. . “I ’ve made a lot of progress..somebody look up to me for what I ’ve done." “I ’m trying hard to put them [mother qualities] to use and let other people know that I do have them. . and “my kids will be proud of me”. In 89 this study, transformative Ieaming in which parent training contributed to a major change in the parent’s lives, was described as an overarching explanatory theme in parent responses (First 8 Way, 1995). Implicit Theory Dweck et al. (1991) examined individuals with similar abilities who set either learning or performance goals and suggested that these individuals have different implicit theories about the nature of the intelligence. Individuals who are entity theorists view intelligence (and other traits, attributes) as a fixed entity which cannot be changed and over which they have no control (Hong, Chiu, Dweck, 8 Sacks, 1997). Those individuals who are incremental theorists view intelligence (and other qualities) as incrementally responsive and malleable to an individual’s effort (Brophy, 1998; Hong et al., 1997; Levy, Stroessner, 8 Dweck, 1998). This implicit theory of intelligence has been used to explain why some individuals favor performance goals over Ieaming goals and vice versa (Dweck 8 Leggett, 1988). In addition, the implicit theories have been applied to social domains and generalized to external attributes. When individuals adapt entity theories in which other’s attributes are viewed as fixed or uncontrollable, they are predisposed towards judgment goals which influence behavior (low initiation and resistance towards change), cognition (rigid, over-simplified thinking), and affect (contempt) (Dweck 8 Leggett, 1988). Furthermore, individuals who are entity theorists are more 90 likely to give up in the face of difficulties (Dweck 8 Leggett, 1988). Recent research has implicated the role of entity theory as predictive of biases towards social stereotyping (Levy et al., 1998), prejudices, evaluative processing (Hong et al., 1997), and increased emphasis on evaluative processes rather than processing and contextualizing factors which mediate behavior (Heyman 8 Dweck, 1998). When individuals adapt incremental theories in which others’ attributes are viewed as malleable, they are predisposed toward developmental goals which influence behavior (mastery oriented goal pursuit), cognition (process analysis), and affect (empathy) (Dweck 8 Leggefl,1988) Hong et al. (1997) used three domain-general items of implicit person theory to measure a diverse set of skills as follows: 1. People can do things differently, but the important part of who they are can’t really be changed. 2. The kind of person someone is, is something very basic about them and it can’t be changed very much. 3. Everyone is one kind of person and there is not much that can be done to really change that. (Hong et al., 1997, p. 303). In order to depict a unitary theme, avoid monotonous repetition, highly compelling, and socially desirable responses only three items pertaining to entity theory were used while explicitly incremental theorist type items were not depicted. Apparently, this practice was well substantiated by research (Chiu, Dweck, Tong, 8 Fu, 1997; Hong et al., 1997). Furthermore, disagreement with the entity theory items can be taken to 91 represent an incremental theory (Chiu et al., 1997). Previous validation studies have found the scale to have high internal reliability, test-rest reliability, and construct validity (Hong et al., 1997). Furthermore, the scale does not correlate with measures of intelligence, academic aptitude, self esteem, optimism regarding human nature, positivity about the self and others, and ideological rigidity (Hong et al., 1997), self presentation concerns, and political attitudes (conservatism and liberalism scales) (Chiu et al., 1997). These findings suggest that the items are not confounded with these other constructs. A more indirect method of assessing implicit theories would be to ask individuals to explain positive or negative behaviors. For example, individuals would be told “Alexis stole some bread from the bakery shop” and then asked to make causal attributions by completing the following sentence stern: “This probably occurred because...” (Hong et al., 1997). It has been found that entity theorists generate significantly more personality trait responses such as “Alexis is a thief’, “Alexis was dishonest” while incremental theorists tend to generate more process oriented, psychological state explanations such as “Alexis was hungry’, “Alexis was desperate” (Hong et al., 1997). Chiu et al. (1997) studied the association among implicit theories (entity or incremental) and moral beliefs (duty based or rights-based). The authors found that entity theory and moral character were associated with a preference for duty-based moral beliefs which also served to maintain 92 the status quo. In other words, these individuals view other people’s moral character as fixed, consider duty as the basis to judge morally right behavior, are less tolerant of deviance, preserve the status quo, and, are more likely to sanction behavior, use punishment, and not reward appropriate conduct (Chiu et al., 1997). Incremental theories were associated with a preference for rights-based moral beliefs which served to allow, promote, and guide change. Individuals who are incremental theorists view other people’s moral character as malleable, shapeable, cultivated, and improvable; consider fundamental principles and rights as the basis to judge behavior; and, are more likely to use positives, appreciate appropriate behavior, use more negotiation and education, and use less punishment. Within the conduct domain, entity theorists were more likely to use directives while incremental theorists were more likely to use negotiation and assurance when talking to children with undesirable behavior. The influence of implicit theories are likely transferable in parent training and behavioral outcomes. Using the Chiu et al. (1997) measures of morality theories as a framework, the following statements may be viewed as equivalent in the parent training framework: 1. My child’s behavior is something very basic about him/her and it can’t be changed much. 2. Whether my child follows directions, obeys instructions, and acts appropriately or not is deeply ingrained in his/her personality. It cannot be changed much. 3. There is not much that can be done to change my child’s behavioral traits (e.g. oppositional, aggressive, defiant) (adapted from Chiu et al., 1997) 93 Just as implicit theory of personality is an individual difference factor that mediates automatic attitude activation towards others (Hong et al., 1997), implicit theories may serve as mediators in parent training outcomes. No studies were found which addressed parent’s implicit theories and PMT. The Self Self schemas are defined as domain specific and as “cognitive generalizations about the self, derived from past experience, that organize and guide the process of self-related information contained in the individual’s social experience” (Reeve, 1996, p. 139). These self schemas serve several valuable and useful functions: a) promote the ability to encode, evaluate, and retrieve information; b) provide structure for quick and confident judgments; 0) allow flexible adaptation to information processing goals; d) enhance sensitivity and attunement to schema relevant information; e) direct close attention to and favor information relevant to that domain; and, consequently, f) provide individuals with the ability to exercise the skills/abilities when needed (Brophy, 1998; Cross 8 Markus, 1994). Individuals who have developed self schemas are said to be schematic in a domain. Schematic individuals recognize that they have a particular ability (in that particular domain) and believe that this ability is of critical importance which together produce their self schemas (Cross 8 Markus, 1994). Individuals are said to be aschematic when they do not possess a self schema in a particular domain (Cross 8 Markus, 1994). 94 Aschematic individuals do not recognize that they have a particular ability (in that particular domain) and do not believe that this ability is of critical importance, (or at least at lower levels) (Cross 8 Markus, 1994). It has been postulated that aschematic individuals may be less likely to anticipate, recognize, and employ the relevant abilities and strategies needed for completing tasks, especially when tasks are novel and require considerable structuring (Cross 8 Markus, 1994). Self schemas have been measured through self ratings on domain specific direct questions as follows: How good are you at analytical problem solving? How good are you at logical problem solving? How important is analytical problem solving to you? How important is logical problem solving to you? (Cross 8 Markus, 1994) 99°F)?" The function and organization of self schemas have been examined in several domains such as problem solving and competence (above), dependence-independence, gender roles, shyness, depression, career orientation, and Type A personality (Cross 8 Markus, 1994). Students who were schematic and aschematic performed equally well on a problem solving task yet demonstrated variable judgements regarding perceptions of facility and enjoyment of the task (Cross 8 Markus, 1994). No attention has been given to the consequences of schematicity in parenting performance with children with disruptive behaviors. Yet, these same questions can be directly addressed through domain specific, simple modifications: 95 How good are you at controlling your child’s behavior? How good are you at managing your child’s behavior? How important is it to you to control your child’s behavior? How important is it to you to manage your child’s behavior? PPN.‘ These questions may also be modified to be even more domain specific or setting specific (e.g. how good are you at controlling behavior in the home?) Self schemas change through discrepant feedback and/or through future oriented self conceptions which are called possible selves (Cross 8 Markus, 1994). Markus and Nurius (1986) developed the concept of possible selves which are cognitive representations of oneself in the future" (Brophy, 1998, p. 216). Possible selves represent those selves the person could become, would like to become, or is afraid to become (Cross 8 Markus, 1994, p.424). With the creation of more vivid and elaborated possible selves, individuals tend to perform better toward desired outcomes (Cross 8 Markus, 1994). Furthermore, being able to view oneself as “successful" within a particular domain in the future may also help reduce anxiety or worry during the activities (Cross 8 Markus, 1994). Integrating Inner Motivational Resources Many of these inner resources can be organized within the expectancy x value model of motivation. The expectancy x value model predicts four primary student responses to classroom activities. First, students react with engagement when they see value to a task and they are confident they can do it (Brophy, 1998). Second, students react with dissembling when they recognize the value but do not feel capable of 96 meeting the task demands (Brophy, 1998). Third, students respond through evasion when task value is low yet they feel capable of successful performance (Brophy, 1998). Finally, students respond with rejection when task value perceptions are low coupled with low success expectations (Brophy, 1998). These same student strategies for responding may be applicable to parents engaged in parent training programs (Table 12). Dissembling parents and evading parents may differ only in program completion; for example, dissembling parents may complete the program but find excuses such as time constraints while evading parents may simply drop out. Obvious parallels can be drawn among the various motivational aspects of self efficacy theory, attribution theory, personal control beliefs, goal setting, value, etc. For example, Pintrich and De Groot (1990) reported that self reports of self-efficacy and value perceptions (interesting and important course work) were positively related to student performance on classroom English and Science assignments. Many causal attribution theorists have focused on individual’s causal attributions regarding performance on tasks and have neglected value aspects inherent in many tasks (Brophy, 1998). The interdependent relationship with self-efficacy and personal control expectations and parent causal attributions addressing task value aspects of motivation may be revealed by answers to the following open ended questions: 97 .89 {seem 52.. 85m}. .902 8.593 35883 .9 beam .205 among; ”:29QO 9:0... 9 9:58. o~=9ocom 9 29:. ”m9_>=om 96.6 octane m>zom EoEmmmmcm GEE 92 :09 $3333 £3, =$525.0: new BEE ”8:99.88 ho mums: 9099a 0.538.0me So 9:306 L9 xm: :9; ”3:039:02 Eomnm 8 992.8 -E ”8255,: 9: moon 858$ :9: So @5306 .9 xm: :9; 6956 E cozmgofima .mE_:_E 20.98me 33 =9: m:_m> mcozmfimaxw 26.. $825 mcozaoohmn. o2m> x >oc90maxw :9: 9 o9m_mm mm ten. 9 ucficoommm .9 $5995 Ewan oo~_mo£oa>I NV 92m... 98 1. What benefits do I expect to derive from engaging in this task? 2. Why do I seek lavoid opportunities to learn about parent management? 3. What does parent management training do/fail to do for me? (adapted from Brophy, 1998; Brophy, 1999). Subjective task value may have three major components as described by Eccles and Wigfield (1985). The first, attainment value, involves achieving success for enhancing self concept or fulfilling achievement, power, or prestige needs. Thus, parents may sustain interest and appreciate PMT involvement in order to meet certain personal needs. The second, intrinsic or interest value, involves engaging in tasks because of the enjoyment of involvement. Translating this concept of intrinsic value into PMT involvement, parents may also engage in PMT because of the social aspects. These parents are interacting with a reference peer group where they can make social comparisons. The third component, utility value, involves engaging in the task in order to reach certain goals. In the PMT case, this may involve the parental perception that PMT will aid in meeting their goals as explicitly articulated at the start of therapy, though sometimes, implicitly addressed. These three task value components may be reflected in the following statements: 1. Involvement in PMT will help me learn how to manage my child’s behavior and help me feel better about myself. 2. Involvement in PMT will help me Ieam how to manage my child’s behavior and have better control and power. 3. Involvement in PMT will help me Ieam how to manage my child’s behavior and other’s won’t look down on me so much, anymore. 4. Involvement in PMT will be interesting and possibly fun, as I will meet other parents in similar situations. 5. Involvement in PMT will be useful in helping me to reach my goals. 99 F” Involvement in PMT will be helpful in improving the quality of our lives. I think that what I am Ieaming in this group is useful for me to know. I think that what we are learning in this class is interesting. It Is important for me to Ieam what Is being taught In these groups. 0. I like what I am learning In these groups. (Pintrich 8 De Groot, 1990) doefl Support for considering the value aspects of motivation within PMT contexts and antisocial populations comes from the work of (Kazdin et al., 1997; Prinz 8 Miller, 1994). Kazdin et al. (1997) examined barriers to child and/or parent treatment and found that parent perceptions of treatment irrelevance was associated with premature termination from therapy. Prinz and Miller (1994) found that parents who had been given opportunities to discuss feelings about being in therapy (and other personal issues) showed lower drop out rates. Results from both of these studies suggest that parent perceptions of value may be predictive of PMT outcomes. Research has effectively established the interrelationships among sense of efficacy or expectations for success and the following: causal attributions for success and failure, learning versus performance goals, selection of strategies for accomplishing tasks, managing failure, and handling frustration (Brophy, 1999). Within specific content domains, research has established relationships among various motivational variables and perceptions of competence. For example, in comparison to students who rated their competence as low in mathematics, students who rated their competence highly also demonstrated learning and mastery 100 goals, more positive emotions, greater enjoyment, and fewer negative emotions (Stipek, Salmon, Giwin, 8 Kazemi, 1998). Within the parenting domain, a more pertinent study finding involves the association between mothers’ feelings of self efficacy and attributions of personal controllability, i.e., beliefs about their ability to impact child misbehaviors (Geller 8 Johnston, 1995). Goal theorists have extensively researched motivational factors (expectations, self-efficacy, attributions) and their influence on individual’s engagement in tasks and ultimate levels of achievement success (Brophy, 1998). This inter-relationship and mediating influence of goals can be seen in the functional definition of goals: Serving as filtering mechanisms for beliefs about the self and subsequent action and as setting the stage for eliciting motivational cognitions (such as values, expectations, attributions, and self- evaluations), particular cognitive operations (mental representations, monitoring, evaluation of responses and strategic thinking), and affects about the self, and the tasks and others. (Lemos, 1996, p.152) Research has begun to establish the interrelationship among classes of goals as follows: goals play a critical role in setting up individual’s attributions; self schemas and possible selves are beginning to be discussed in relation to people’s classes of goals; the nature of the relationship between self efficacy, related variables (competency). and goals is being explored (as noted above) (Dweck, 1996). Research has yet to establish empirically the connection between the value, interest, and appreciation aspects of motivation and 101 engagement in domain specific Ieaming activities as well as ideas about engagement in activities (Brophy, 1999). Theoretically, (Dweck, 1996) suggests that attitudes (including values and evaluations) translate into behavior when housed within goal structures; the more value one gives to something the more it has the power to drive goals. At the very least, these motivational aspects and value aspects may interact to predict PMT outcomes. Ideally, all of these aspects comprise the major elements of a cognitive theory of motivation and may be structurally combined in a complex motivational schema which impacts on PMT outcomes. In a model, described as Schema Mediators, the present dissertation would like to build on the above conceptualizations of inner motivational resources and value aspects and incorporate the various components into an integrated model with interrelated parts. Like students who are high in motivation to learn, parents who are high in motivation to Ieam may also possess a motivated Ieaming schema which is triggered/activated whenever they enter a PMT program (Brophy, 1998). Those parents with most successful outcomes, ideally would possess a motivation to Ieam schema described as follows: A network of connected insights, skills, values, and dispositions, that enable [parents] to understand what it means to engage in [PMT] activities with the intention of accomplishing their Ieaming goals and with awareness of the strategies that they use in attempting to do so. (Brophy, 1998, p. 14) Furthermore, these schema are instantly accessible, guide information processing and problem solving, are accumulated from prior knowledge 102 and experience, and tend to be domain or situation-specific (Brophy, 1999). Those with more elaborated and better developed motivational and cognitive schemas are more likely to appreciate and benefit form engagement in more abstract and complex Ieaming activities (Brophy, 1999) Just as Brophy (1998) points out that the same principles that apply to student motivation and achievement, apply to teacher motivation and professional challenges, the same may apply to parent motivation and PMT outcomes. Furthermore, it has been suggested that “motivated functioning in [their respective] domain[s] is schema-driven” (Brophy, 1999). It would be worthwhile to examine how these motivational resources combine to influence parent outcomes in PMT. Two additional, final points need to be addressed. First, many of the concepts noted above have been essentially dichotomized and described as mutually exclusive dimensions such as in task (learning) versus ego (performance) goal orientations even though evidence suggests the two goals are interdependent (Ames 8 Archer, 1988; Meece 8 Holt, 1993). Other examples involve studies which examine high efficacy versus low efficacy, internal versus external attributions. Unlike the above studies, future studies plan on evaluating the individual motivational inner resources on a continuum and acknowledge that an entire spectrum of motivational factors may exist in evaluating various levels of the concepts. 103 Second, the various parent motivational resources throughout the PMT interventions may not be static constructs. It has been argued that “motivation in therapy is not a personal characteristic, stable trait, something a person does or does not have, or a driving force channeled by biology and experience” (Kanfer, 1996, p. 2). As such, it is quite possible that parent motivations in PMT may be subject to a process of continual “refinement” and adjustment to environmental conditions over the course of treatment. Research findings suggest that as parents are involved in the PMT process, their cognitive processes may go through various phases and changes as they work to cope with their child’s disruptive behavior (Spitzer, Webster-Stratton, 8 Hollinsworth, 1991). Johnston (1996) speculates that as parents progress through these phases, different cognitions (including motivations) may be central at different times. Furthermore, parent motivations, independent of PMT participation, may also be subject to the same processes. Consequently, parent motivational resources and value aspects may change. Two areas of research have been drawn upon for this dissertation. One has to do with traditionally examined predictors which are usually consistent with demographic information about parents and children and their impact on PMT outcomes. The second has to do with conditions “inside” the parent. The concern here is predominantly with the parent preferred style of interacting with their child, nature of parent goals and values, and degree of responsibility for affording change in their child. This second area can be expanded to include parent 104 motivational resources and values and their influence on outcomes. To consider only those conditions which are internal or external to the parent and child in isolation is insufficient. The challenge is to consider several predictors and eventually find ways of analyzing and predicting influences of various combinations, and to ultimately develop treatment plans for better outcomes. The various nine motivational inner resources (self efficacy, optimistic explanatory style, pessimistic explanatory style, Ieaming goals, self enhancing performance goals, self defeating performance goals and self schemas) in conjunction with valuing aspects (interest, appreciation, importance) are integrated into a smaller number of cohesive motivational schemas in the pilot study below. The pilot study addresses the first question related to motivational resources and provides the impetus for the second question which is examined in the main study: 1. Do the motivational resources and value aspects covary to form central schemas (4 or 5) among individual parents? And if so, 2. Are these schemas predictive of parent training outcomes? 105 PILOT STUDY THE DEVELOPMENT OF THE PARENT MOTIVATION SCHEMA SCALE The Parent Motivation Schema Scale (PMSS) is designed to uncover particular parent motivational aspects and values which may eventually contribute to, if not determine, outcomes in parent management training. Nine of the twelve inner motivational resources are reflected in the PMSS. These cognitive motivational resources include self efficacy, attributions (divided into optimistic and pessimistic explanatory styles), learning goals, performance goals (both self-enhancing and self-defeating), implicit theories, self schemas, and value. The purpose of study 1 was to establish the reliability, internal validity, and factor structure of the PMSS. METHOD Participants The pilot survey participants were 200 parents (168 mothers, 3O fathers, 2 unknown) and their children (78 girls, 113 boys, 9 unknown). Families were 91 % White, 6% Native Canadian, and 1 % Asian Canadian. The children’s mean age at the time of the survey was 9.25 (SD=4.50). The treatment and control participants were 93 parents (69 mothers and 24 fathers) and their children (29 girls and 64 boys). Families were 81 "/0 White and 16 % Native Canadian. The children’s mean age at the point of the treatment study was 8.23 (so=2.35). 106 Parent gender, education, current annual household income and child gender as well as child education for all participants (pilot survey, treatment, and control) are summarized in Tables 13 and 14, respectively. The group of parents who served as participants live in a small, northern, Canadian community (population = 80,000). The participant demographic data is representative of this population. Measure In this first study developing the PMSS, items are adapted from previous factor analysis research. Items are adapted from various instruments with stable factor structures used to assess student motivation aspects including self efficacy (Midgley et al., 1989; Pajares, 1996; Reeve, 1996), goal theory (Ames 8 Archer, 1988; Midgley et al., 1996; Skaalvik, 1997), implicit theory (Cross 8 Markus, 1994; Hong et al., 1997), and values (Brophy, 1999; Pintrich 8 De Groot, 1990). Factor analysis had originally been used to guide the scale construction of each of these motivational aspects within their respective research studies. Within the present study, these items are translated and adapted to fit the specific parenting domain as opposed to the initial content domains of the original studies. The domain specificity of the PMSS items are based on the principal investigator’s clinical experiences with PMT. Parents have offered statements in therapy, written comments in homework assignments, made notes in journals, and reflections in behavioral diaries, which implicate specific motivational resources. The domain specificity of these item responses are generated from 107 Table 13 Demcmraphic Characteristics of Parent Participants in PMSS Pilot Study Pilot PMT Groups Total (n=200) (n=93) (n = 293) Descriptive fl % Ll °/o _I\_l % Parent gender Male 30 15 24 26 54 18 Female 168 84 69 73 230 78 Parent education Elementary 4 2 3 3 7 2 High School 52 26 32 34 81 28 College (Community) 64 32 38 40 100 34 University (undergraduate) 47 24 13 14 60 20 University (graduate) 28 14 5 5 33 1 1 Other 2 1 0 0 2 1 Ethnicity/ Race Native Canadian or Inuit 12 6 15 16 27 9 Asian-Canadian 2 1 0 0 2 1 White / Caucasian 182 91 76 81 252 86 Current Annual Household Income Under $10,000 4 2 10 11 13 4 $10,000 - 14,999 4 2 18 19 21 7 $15,000 — 19,999 3 2 1O 11 11 4 $20,000 - 24,999 11 6 10 11 20 7 $25,000 — 29,999 10 5 1 1 11 4 $30,000 — 34,999 6 3 1 1 7 2 $35,000 - 39,999 12 6 6 6 15 5 $40,000 - 44,999 10 5 3 3 12 4 $50,000 - 54,999 12 6 7 7 19 7 $55,000 - 59,999 11 6 8 9 18 6 $60,000 — 64,999 21 11 4 4 26 9 $65,000 — 69,999 23 12 6 6 3O 10 $70,000 and over 61 31 8 9 67 23 Note. Totals do not sum to 100% due to missing data. 108 Table 14 Demographic Characteristics of Children Whose Parents Completed the Pilot PMSS m=293) fllo_t mtment a_nd Control Totals Characteristic g % fl % N % Child Gender Male 113 57 63 67 172 59 Female 78 39 29 31 105 36 Child Education Preschool 23 12 2 2 24 8 Junior Kindergarten 6 3 10 11 11 4 Senior Kindergarten 11 6 3 3 14 5 Grade 1 20 10 15 16 34 12 Grade 2 11 6 6 6 16 5 Grade 3 16 8 14 15 28 10 Grade 4 12 6 12 13 25 9 Grade 5 15 8 11 12 24 8 Grade 6 9 5 14 15 22 8 Grade 7 12 6 3 3 15 5 Grade 8 7 4 1 1 7 2 Grade 9 16 8 O 0 17 6 Grade 10 10 5 O O 10 3 Grade 11 6 3 O 0 6 2 Grade 12 9 5 0 0 9 3 Grade 13 / OAC O O O 0 5 2 Other 2 1 1 1 2 1 (special education classes) Note. Totals do not sum to 100% due to missing data. 109 these sources as well as from past informal discussions with professionals experienced in the field of PMT. Statements are written to measure parent motivational aspects and values regarding both a) their children’s behavioral difficulties and b) involvement in parent management therapies. Some of the values of parents are transformed into a set of futuristic statements reflecting perceived usefulness, importance, and relevance of PMT. As such, explicit expectations are unavoidably combined with value statements. Response Scale The PMSS is written in English and can be self-administered in approximately 15-35 minutes. Parents are required to rate how they feel about a set of 44 statements, on a scale of 1 to 5, where 1=not true of me at all, 2=just a little true of me, 3= somewhat true of me, 4 =very much true of me, and 5 = extremely true of me. The statements are divided into nine subscales based upon motivational aspects (self efficacy, optimistic explanatory style, pessimistic explanatory style, Ieaming goals, self-enhancing performance goals, self- defeating performance goals, implicit theory, self schema/schematicity, and value). Points earned for responses to each subgroup of statements are combined yielding a total score for each of the motivational aspects subscales. High scores on seven of the subscales indicate corresponding high levels of self efficacy, optimistic explanatory style, pessimistic explanatory style, learning goal orientation, self enhancing performance goal orientation, self defeating performance goal orientation, and value regarding PMT. For these seven 110 subscales, low scores indicate lower levels on these seven motivational resources. The remaining two subscales (implicit theories and self schemas) are essentially dichotomized scales. A high implicit theory score indicates parents may have more of an entity theorist orientation whereas a low implicit theory score suggests more of an incremental theorist orientation. Similarly, a high score on self schemas items indicates a parent who is schematic whereas a low score indicates a parent who is aschematic. These two subtypes are phrased in a positive fashion based on arguments posed in previous research (Chiu et al., 1997; Hong et al., 1997). Items from each of the nine subscales are randomly distributed throughout the scale to reduce the potential for any further biased responding such as selecting the same types of responses for clustered similar items. Several motivational aspects are likely to be present simultaneously with varying degrees of intensity and /or even struggling for dominance. If parent responses within a particular subgroup of motivational aspects are inconsistent, the response pattern is said to be nonconvergent and will not likely load onto the same factor. It is hypothesized that the practical application of parent motivational aspects lie in application to PMT practice and approaches and not so much for the purpose of categorizing or labeling parents. Contributions towards VaMy and Reliability Validity data includes content and construct validity, (convergent and discriminant). Reliability data includes split-half reliability and test-retest reliability. The development of the PMSS is characterized by four main phases: 1) finding 111 evidence to support the item content and content validity; 2) administering the scale to a pilot group; 3) conducting a factor analysis of the scale; and, 4) evaluating the reliability of the scale). Phase 1: Establishing item content: First, the survey designer developed a comprehensive list of potential statements for inclusion in the PMSS. List items were generated by the principal investigator based upon a review of the professional literature on key motivational inner resources and values as well as clinical experiences (both described above). The 55 potential items were randomly ordered in a questionnaire (see Appendix A for Pilot Study: Pilot Survey). Second, individual items of the PMSS were checked for agreement by the principal investigator and a professor who is an active researcher in the area of motivation. The professor identified motivational aspects with corresponding item statements in a checklist fashion (see Appendix A for Pilot Study: Agreement Among Independent Rater Responses). For this portion of the second phase, items were retained or discarded based on the following simple decision rule: a) items in which both raters agreed (100%) were retained and, b) items in which raters did not agree were discussed in order to retain an adequate sample of motivational resources. By retaining only these items in which the independent raters agreed are an adequate and representative sample of the respective motivational aspects, then the scale can be said to have support for content validity (see Appendix A for Pilot Study: Scale Items Listed by Domain). Based on these reviewers’ ratings, items were revised and deleted from the scale and 112 the resulting 44 item scale was pilot tested (see Appendix A for Pilot Study: Final PMSS). Phase 2: Administering the Scale to the Pilot, Treatment, and Control Groups; This second draft was administered to the participants. The validity and reliability of the PMSS was tested on a sample of parents of children who receive services in an out-patient health clinic and/or attend public or separate school boards, each mandated by the Ministry of Health and Ministry of Education, respectively. Parents were asked to read a consent form indicating their voluntary participation in a research project prior to completing the survey (see Appendix B for Pilot Study: Consent Form). These surveys were collected by randomly surveying parents as they attended a clinic or through a mail out survey to clinic members who attended the Communication Disorders Department over the past two years. The PMSS surveys completed by the treatment and control parents were collected during the treatment phase of the study. These surveys were independently analyzed statistically for phases 3 and 4 of the pilot project. On the basis of the interpretability of the results it was decided to pool together the surveys from both samples (see Appendix C for Decisions on Factor Dimensions). Phase 3: Evaluating the Factor Stmcture of the Scale Each of the items contributing to the respective motivational aspects were also summed for a total subscale score. Cronbach alphas were also calculated 113 for these subscale scores. A principal component analysis was conducted on the subscales to adjust for a smaller sample size, if necessary. A principal components analysis was then conducted on the individual 144 PMSS items. A varimax rotation was used to determine the number of factors necessary to describe the data. Pearson correlations were used to assess the stability of the PMSS. Cronbach’s alphas were calculated for the PMSS total and factor scores. Phase 4: Evaluating Reliability of the Scale The Reliability of the PMSS was tested through internal consistency, (coefficient alpha reliability). Alpha reliability analyses were conducted with data from the above parents already participating in therapy in the clinic or school board. Alpha reliability analyses were also conducted by randomly selecting patients with appointments throughout the clinic, to complete the questionnaire. Test-retest reliability analysis of the PMSS was evaluated on a smaller sample, by using separate and combined responses from the treatment and control groups of the research study at the pre- and post- intervals. In this procedure, the same PMSS was administered to the same group of participants on two different occasions and then the two sets of scores were correlated to obtain sets of scores. The number of weeks between the first and second administration of the PMSS ranged from a minimum of 9 weeks to a maximum of 16 weeks. Test re-test reliability coefficients for each of the subscales, total scores, and factor scores were examined. These reliability coefficients indicate the degree of stability (consistency) in scores over time and is also referred to as 114 the coefficient of stability. The recommended reliability level is .80 for research purposes. RESULTS The mean PMSS total score was 126.32 (SD =16.33). The PMSS total scores ranged from 70 to 192. Coefficient alpha for the total items of the PMSS was .87. The subscale mean scores are presented in Table 15. These results demonstrate the ability of the PMSS to reflect variability in parent motivation and provide a heterogeneous sample for principal component analysis of the scales. Table 15 also contains an evaluation of the correlations among the subscales as well as a reliability analysis for each of the subscales which comprise the PMSS. Several subscales are significantly correlated (Table 15). The item reliabilities range from a high of .91 for the Value Subscale to a low of .32 for the Pessimistic Explanatory Style Subscale (Table 15). However, the factor structure of the PMSS did not support the nine subscales reflecting the motivational resource literature (see Appendix C for Principal Component Analysis Results for Nine Factor Solution). The principal component analysis of the individual 144 PMSS items yielded five factors based on using a scree plot decision rule. The elbow rule in the scree plot criteria, supports extracting five factors (Figure 1). In addition, all of these factors have eigenvalues substantially greater than 1 (Table 16). All of the 144 items of the PMSS and their factor loadings are presented in Table 17. Only the items with the highest factor loadings on the five factors are presented in Table 18. 115 90.0.. So.vm . .0629 n ..<> ”Eoszmcow u Iow ”0959: =0__QE_ u as: ”655990 :09 28m 8cmEotoa u Don. meocmzco :59 053 858.950 u de ”a_moa mEEmm. u 04 ”30:35.9 a_bm b92398 029E686 u wmn. ”3:250:99 a_bw boumcmaxm 039E300 n mmo $0850 :60 n mm .4. 56:890. 5 .090: Sn mE9_ 9:..902 N00 90.0 9.0 9.0 :0 90.0 0N0 N00 N00 000 2% 00.0. 00.9 00.9 9N 00.0 00.0 00.0 0N9 00N 2N 0000300 0.00090 N009 N090 0N0 00.0 00.09 00.0 5.8 00.N 00.9 8.8 000.2 0.888 N9 3. 09 3 0N NN 0m 0 9 0N 00_0> 505082 0N : N 0 0 0 0 N 0 9 23> 52552 3: 3 N 0 0 0 0 N 0 0 0&9. .0 .02 00.9 30. N00- .000 .000 .000 .N0 000 N00- 829 00.9 90.0- .NN.0 00.0- 000 N0. .000 .30 26005050.. 00. 9 .0N.0 .0N.0 9.0- . 90.0 .0 90. N00- 8:09: 5:05. 00. 9 .000 .0N.0 .0N.0 .0N.0 L 9.0 mm L 0.80 :08 00. 9 00.0 .N0.0 :m 9.0- 30. om \ 280 Ea 00,— 00.0 .0N.0 .0N.0 0.86 0558.. 00.9 90.0- 5.0- 0.5 035588 009 .000 035 0005000 00. 9 68% =8 .909 3<> row as: mméa 00-08 6.. mma 30 mm 0.888 mmsE 9: So 906 mm 5:89:90 .o £0 .95 ___nm__9 uclm .mcozmSoo 98:90 0:09: .8590 .3399; co=m>=oE 65 dcoElm 30:99.00 or 9an 116 Scree Plot 10-.--._m- __ _ __ #_ .__ -0 ._ ,. . 8‘ 6“ 4.. 2I (U 7 ‘- ....... .......... ......... - ‘ r-rv . La- 0 ' a. g. L-:‘ — _. ">o0:0: 0.0....0 >E 0:...0.€00 .0 0000 E0 . .00 000.0 0N .0 000.0 000.0 00.0- 00:03.0 05 0.0:0: 0:0 .00 .055: .00 .055 50 0.00.. :00 _ .0000E 050.: .0 0:0 :_ 0. 0.20 >E :0:>> .0.. N000- 3 .0 00N.0 mm .0 000.0 .E00. 0. 00... _ 003000: 0. 0:03 0:0.0000 €0.00 >E 00 0.003 _ :0000. 50E 0: .- .00 00.0 :00 000.0 000.0 30.0 .0.0E>:0 .:0:E 00 0:. :0 5500 x00. 0:03 0.050 0:0 .0.>0:00 0.0..:0 >E 0.0:0: 0. 30: E00. 0:. 0.0: _...s 0:0.0000 €0.00 5 €0E0>.0>:. .00 000.0- 300. 000.0 03.0 000.0 .055 0E 000.0E .. :05... .00: 0:0; 0.:0.0000 €0.00 00... ._..s . .5 000.0 0NNO- .00.0- 0000 000.0 0:30.000 000 .00.0.0.. .0. .0 000.00. 0.050 :05 0:. 0. E9505 0.0:. 0. 0003 0.5.0000 €0.00 05 0:.0:0.0.00:3 .00 03.0 03.0 00.0 03.0 30.0 0.0.0: 30:0. ..:0.0 _ 0:.50E00 E00. _ :05... 0:0.0000 €0.00 5 3.0000000 .00E .00. ...>>. ...~ 000.0 000.0 .0000 000.0. 000.0 0000.0 €0.00 5 €000. 0: ._..s 0:3 E00. 0. 0E .0. €0000E. 0. ._ .v .00.0- 0N0.0 000.0 ~000- thd .0.000 >E :000. 050.0: 5 5.00: 0: .._.s 0:0.0000 €0.00 5 €0E0>.o>:_ .00 00.0. 0.0.0 0.0.0- 8.0. 30.0 .00>.. .00 .0 3:030 05 0:.>0.0E. 5 5.0.0: 00 __..s 0:0.0000 €0.00 :. €0E0>.0>:. .0. 000.0- 000.0 000.0- 000.0- 80.0 .._00>E 3000 .0500 .00. 0E 0.0... 0:0 .0.>0:00 0.0..:0 >E 0.0:0: 0. 30: E00. 0E 0.0: __..s 0:0.0000 €0.00 :. €0E0>.0>:_ .0 0.0.0- 000.0- 00 .0 9.0.0 .0.-.0 000.90_E .0 .0. 0 050E . .. :0>0 E0.. E00. _....> . 0:02. 0.5.0000 €0.00 00... . .mm 30.0- 000.0 0 00.0 0.0.0. 2.0.0 0000.0 0005 5 0:.E00. E0 _ .055 0v... =05 . .0. 000.0- 000.0 30.0- 30.0 «3.0 0:000:50 .0_.E.0 :. 9:0.00 .050 .00E :05. 00 .:0. 20.0000 0:0 05.00.95 0: 0.00? 0000.0 €0.00 5 €0E0>.0>:. .0.. 30.0 08.0. 0000 N0 :9 0.0.0 .2000 00 0.00; 000.0 .00.00 0 0. 0.00. 0.00; _ .00; .05 0.0.5. .9 000.0- .000 .000 .000. 000.0 0000.0 .00.00 0. 0.00. 0.08 _ .003 9... 0.00; . .3 000.0 000.0 00.0 0N0.0. 03.0 0500.95 0: 0.002. 000.0 €0.00 0 5 E00. 0.003 _ .055 .05 0.55. <- 02". .30”. 00.: .- 03.0 0:0 0E9. 005003 .900”. .mmmuz. @020. 05.0. :0_S_0m 5.00002“. 0:000:00 x0E..0> .0. 0.050004 .900“. 0:0 E9. .0 EmEEam n.- 900-.- 119 000.0 000.0 000.0 003.0 030.0 000.0 003.0 003.0 000.0 00.20 000.0 00.20- 000.0- 003.0- 000.0- 00..0- 000.0- 000.0 003.0 000.0 00.0.0 000.0 «00.0 00.20 000.0 003.0 000.0 000.0- 003.0 300.0- 00.20- 03.0 000.0- 3.00.0 0.00.0 003.0 000.0 03.0 000.0 003.0 000.0 0 ..md 000.0 33.0 000.0 000.0- 000.0 000.0- 000.0- 003.0- 000.0 000.0- 003.0- 300.0- 000.0 000.0 003.0 00 _..o 000.0- 003.0- 000.0- 000.0 0 3.0 000.0 30.0 000.0 000.0 30.0 000.0- 000.0- 000.0 000.0 000.0 30.0- 0 00.0 000.0- 000.0 000.0 000.0 000.0 03.0 300.0 003.0- 000.0- 000.0- .:00E 000:0:0 0: ..000 .. 0:0 .0:.E.: .0000 0.00: 00> 0:.50E00 0. .0320: 0.0::0 >5. .00 00500.0. 05:0.» 05...: .00. .0.>0:0: 0.0::0 0E 00:0:0 0. 0:00 0: :00 .0:. :00E .0: 0. 0.0:: .00 .:00E 000:0:0 0: ..:00 ._ .0. 0: 0:3 .0 ..00 0 0. .0: .0 000.0 0.00 0:0 .0:0..00..05 000:0 .0:0..00..0 030:0. 0..:0 0E .0:.0:>> .0 ..0.>0:0: 0.0_.:0 0E 05.0:0: .0 E0 _ 0000 30: 30:0 0E .0. .0:. 0052.00 0:0.0000 €0.00 00.: 0.003 _ .0 .0.:0.00 .050 :05 .0.>0:0: 0500:0E .0 .050: E0 _ .05 050000 005.50 .0.:0.00 0E. 0.00.00 0.0 0:? 0050.0. .0:.0 0E 30:0 0. 00... 0.00.5 . .03 .0.:0.00 .0:.0 0:. :05 .0.>0:0: 05.0:0: .0 .000: E0 _ .0:. ..0000. 000.0 €0.00 0E 30:0 0. 00... 0.002, _ .0 . 000:0 0.000.. ..000 _ 9.: 0008. 0.90 o. 0:. .0. 20:005. 0. ._ .050: .< .0. .0050: 00E 0.0000 .0:.0 .0:; 0. 0000.0 5 E05 0500000.0 0:0 0E0: .0 0050.0.E 050.0E .0000 05:. .0.03 0:... .00 .0E .0000 0.55 0.050 .0:? .0000 00E00:00 E0 _ .00...>..00 000.0 5 0.00.0...00 _ :0:>> .00 .8. 0 9... 00.0.02 .0000 00E00:00 E0 . ..00 05.00 0.050:0 0:0 0:000 5 0..:0 >E 5.3 :0:>> .00 .0.:0.00 .0:.0 00 :03 00 00 .0: 00 >05 00:? 00: .00. 0.00.00 .00 .00.0:0 ..05 00 0:0 0E 0. :0.0: .0.:0000 .0 :00 0E 0>0: :00 _ .0:. €05.50 E0 .. 00 .:000.000 .:0.. 0:. 5.3 00500 0: :00 .0: 000.0 .0 :0..00..0 0:. .0 000000: 05:0 0500.0. .0:.>.00 0.00 0..:0 22 .00 0.00.-50 .00E 050: 0. 0:0 .0 0: :0:>> 0..:0 0E 0. :000.5 .00 :00 . .0.. 2.00. _ :0:>> .0 .00 0. .0:; :0 0000. 0E00 0>0: 0:0 00.00.0..0 0E00 30:0. _ .0:. .00. . .A:0.0:0 .0__0E. 0000.0 0:000 5 0.0: 0:0 0>..00.0.0 0E000: 0..:0 0E ._ .0 00.0:0: 0: :00 .. .0: ..0 E.: .00 .0:. 0:.50E00 2.0000 0. 0.0:. .0000E .055: .0 0:0 5 0. 0..:0 >E :0:>> .3 .0E 0. .:0000E. 0. 0.50:0 .0.>0:0: 0.0::0 0E 0500:0E .0 0000 E0 . .00 0..:00 0. 0.00.: 120 Table 18 Factor Loadings for Varimax Orthoqona_l Five-Factor Solution Items Load Factor 1. 7. I think that what I would learn in a parent group would be interesting. 0.843 44. I would like what I could learn in parent groups. 0.839 13. I think that what I would learn in a parent group would be useful for me to know. 0.815 43. Involvement in parent groups would be interesting and possibly fun. 0.812 18. I will like what I am learning in these groups. 0.775 23. I like parent session’s work I will learn from even if I make a lot of mistakes. 0.757 2. Involvement in parent sessions will help me learn how to handle my child’s behavior. 0.728 16. Involvement in parent sessions will be helpful in improving the quality of our lives. 0.721 20. Involvement in parent sessions will be useful in helping me reach my goals. 0.717 4. It is important for me to learn what will be taught in parent groups. 0.699 21. I will feel most successful in parent sessions when I learn something I didn’t know before. 0.644 38. Understanding the parent session’s work is more important to me than others looking at my behavior and approving. 0.598 31. I will like parent session's work best when it makes me thing. 0.573 37. Involvement in parent sessions will help me learn how to handle my child’s behavior and others won't look down on me so much, anymore. 0.457 Factor 2: 40. When my child is in one of his/her moods. I can figure out what set him/her off, and handle the situation. 0.766 30. I am good at controlling my child’s behavior and this is important to me. 0.747 39. I am good at managing my child’s behavior and this is important to me. 0.740 11. When my child is in one of his/her moods, there is usually something that set him off but it can be handled. 0.720 8. If my child became disruptive and noisy in public places (malls. church). I feel that I know some strategies and have some ideas on what to do. 0.697 5. When I really try. I can get through to my child when he or she is being most difficult. 0.641 29. My child gets defying. refusing, angry because of the situation or place but can be calmed with the right approach. 0.592 27.l am confident that I can have my son or daughter listen and do their chores. 0.413 Factor 3: 36. Parents feel bad when they do not do as well as other parents. 0.695 32. When with my child in public and she/he’s acting out. I am concerned about looking like a fool. 0.644 35. When I participate in group activities. I am concerned about what others think about me. 0.567 25. The worst thing about making mistakes at home and discussing them in groups is what other people may notice. 0.519 19. At home. it is important for me to avoid looking like I can't handle things. 0.477 Factor 4: 9. I would like to show my parent group leader, that I am better at handling behavior than the other parents. 0.821 17. I would like to show my other relatives who are parents (my parents, siblings. cousins) that I am better at managing behavior than other parents. 0.777 6. I would like parent sessions activities that let me show how good I am at handling my child’s behavior. 0.471 Factor 5: 3. Whether my child follows directions, obeys instructions. and acts okay or not is a part of who he is. It can't be changed much. 0.780 12. My child's behavior is something very basic about him/her and it can’t be changed much. 0.735 26. There is not much that can be done to change my child's behavior (e.g. hitting. yelling, refusirE). 0.702 121 Together, the five rotated factors accounted for 49.22% of the variance (Table 16). Factor 1 consisted of 10 value items and 5 Ieaming goal items. This rotated factor containing a total of 15 items, accounted for 20.16% of the variance. Factor 2 consisted of 8 items (3 optimistic explanatory style, 3 self efficacy, and 2 schematicity) which accounted for 11.24% of the variance. Factor 3 consisted of 5 items (4 self defeating performance goals and 1 self enhancing performance goal) which accounted for 10.08% of the variance. Factor 4 consisted of 3 items (3 self enhancing performance goals) and accounting for 4.20 of the variance. Factor 5 consisted of all 3 implicit theory items accounting for 3.54% of the variance. Chronbach’s alphas for the factor structure of the PMSS vary (Table 19). The Factor 1 coefficient score is excellent (.93). Coefficient alpha for the Factor 2 score is good (.83). Coefficient alpha for the Factor 3 and 4 scores are adequate (.68 and .71, respectively). Coefficient alpha for the Factor 5 score is also adequate (.72). Pre- and post- PMT motivational subscale measures are significant for all subscales when control and treatment groups are collapsed into total participants (Table 20). However, correlations of the motivational subscales from pre- to post— PMT are higher for the control group than for the treatment group (Table 21). Similarly, pre — and post- PMT subscale measures are significant for all motivational factors when control and treatment groups are collapsed and significant for all factors, except the implicit theory factor when viewed separately (Table 22). These results suggest greater consistency in motivational subscales 122 mud id mod 86 mod 8:24. 852%:ma and no.0 nod and mad mo=___nm__cm 93?. E9. EN mmN and «fim 3.9 5:350 235% 5+ wmh E. E o FNN mmdm cmos. 28m 9 m? vm mm mm mo:_m> E:E_xm_2 F N o NF 5 mo:_m> E:E_:__2 m m m w m? 9:9. ..o 8952 m2“. So... 925 0?... 95 839.630 208m“. swans mmsa 9: co 25: 3; __m 8 8:28 583-82”. .2825 xmetm> .9 823580 9. 2an 123 .< x6083. 0. 02m: Em mEog on... .902 #000 000.0- 000.0- 000.0 03.0 .. 50.0 050.0- 0.0.0 500.0- -.<> N000 500.0- N5P.0- 00.4.0- 00N.0- 0 3.0 500.0 05N.0 100 000.0 300- 03.0 N000- 50N.0 0.0.0 500.0 ms: 000.0 000.0 0000- $0.0 00—..0 . 00..0- w0n. 0N0.0 550.0- N00.0- 05 ..0.- 0Nv.0- n.0n. No.00 0 50.0- 005.0 500.0- 0-_ 0 55.0 NNN.0 000.0 mun. 00¢.0 00¢.0 mwO ..N0.0 mm 6.3—..00 0N0.0 0N0.0 50N.0- 050.0- 00.20 0NP.0 050.0- NN0.0 005.0 -_<> 000 N0 .. .0- 555.0 500.0- 000.0 005.0 «40.0 0 ..0.- 100 N50 0000 000.0 ..F...0- 00.0.0 090 00..0- n..2. 055.0 03.0 N00.0- 00..0- 05_..0 00.0 m0n. 000.0 505.0 005.0- 005.0 00N.0 00m 000.0 003.- 3N0 00N.0 0.. 0N0 5VN.0- 00v.0- mun. 000.0 ..0v.0 me 000.0 mm HEM. -.<> 100 n..)._ 00a m0n. 0.. mun. mmO mm 00:02.0.)— :o_.m>_.o.>_ tan. -o.n. him-.00.”. AmNHCV E.:—..00 new A hmncv “cm—5500...? co. Emcmoi .:2n. .32-0000 55?. -90 $5305. 00002.22 0095 2200.930 0N 0.00.... 124 50.0.... .3. v m .. . mo. v m .. .00:_0> u -.<> ”3.000;.0500 n ..00 00.805 0.2.08. u as: 00000.00 0.000 0.000 0000.50.00 u 00a. 00.000500 :00. 0.000 000085000 u won. .0.0o0 0:.E00. u 0.. ”0.30 09000.90 0.0.8.0000 u mun. ”0:003:00. 0.30 09000.90 000.8000 n 000 $00050 ..00 u mm .< 50:03.4 0. 0000.. 0.0 0800. 0:... .062 005.0 50N0- 0050- 03.0 505.0 034.0 000.0 000.0 000.0 -.<> 00.00 0NNO- 03.0 59.0- 000.0 0¢N0 5¢N0 000.0 100 v0N0 0..0.0 0N00 0050- 520 N050 N0...0- n..>.. 005.0 000.0 0500- 000.0- 00...0 3.0.0 m0n. N000 000.0 03.0- ..0...0 00_..0 00m 5000 000.0 00...0 0..N.0 0.. N500 5.. ..0- ....0.0- mun. 050.0 #300 wwO 0.00 mm .73 100 as: w0n. D0n. 0.. mun. 0wO mm 0000200.). 00:026.). ._-.>.n.-0._n. ._..>_n.-00n. .00.... 0:00.2th :0 .2 E0620 52m. -0000 0:0 ....2n. 0:. 0050005. 0000282 0:080 000000.50 ..N 0.00... 125 Table 22 Correlations amonlMotivation Schema pre- PMT and post- PMT Program for Treatment (n=37 ) and Control (n=29) Post Pre-Motivation Motivation LV IMC PGD PGE IT .Fl/LT. Learning Value 0414* Se/OES/Sch 0.026 0.594" Performance Goal/Defeating 0.037 0.263 0.510“ Performance Goal/Enhancing -0.139 0.221 0.531“ 0.698“ lmplicitTheory -0.296 0.074 0.117 0.187 0.267 Control Learning Value 0.713“ Se/OES/Sch -0.872** 0.714“ Performance Goal/Defeating -0.153 -0.052 0595* Performance Goal/Enhancing 0.102 0.257 0.604 0.826“ Implicit Theory 075 0.009 0.427 -0.492 0.425 M Learning Value 0.647“ Se/OES/Sch -0.235 0.582“ Performance Goal/Defeating 0.012 0.176 0.533“ Performance Goal/Enhancing -0.023 0.223 0.559“ 0.732“ Implicit Theory -o.24o 0.073 0.220 -o.002 0.316* *p<.05 **p<.01 126 for the control group (Table 20 and 21). In fact, test-retest reliabilities of the PMSS indicate highest reliability coefficients for the control group (Table 23). Test — retest r_‘s ranged from .905 for self enhancing performance goals (control group) to .421 for implicit theory (treatment group). DISCUSSION The results of the present pilot study indicate that the various nine motivational inner resources can be integrated into a smaller number of motivational schemas. The results also suggest that the PMSS is a psychometrically sound instrument for measuring parent motivation regarding child behavior and participation in PMT programs. Study 1 establishes the factor structure of the PMSS. This study suggests that the PMSS assesses parent motivational aspects into five principal components/factors. A reliability analysis of these factors indicates that the structure is sufficient for the five factors. The variable item reliability results likely reflect the number of items which comprise the respective subscales as well as elements of noise in the data. In future, some items from the scale may be excluded because of a lack of correlation or factor structure. The five factors are comprised of specific motivational aspects and can be named: (1. Learning Value; 2. Internal Motivational Controls; 3. Performance Goals-Self Defeating; 4. Performance Goals- Self Enhancing; and, 5. Implicit Theory). Factor 1 consists of scale items from two motivational resources (value and learning goals) which represent parent’s positive hopes and expectations. This more existential, transcendental, and idealistic factor can be labeled, 127 Table 23 Test-Retest Reliabilitv Coefficients gre- PMT and post- PMT Program for Treatment (n=37 )and Control (n=29) Scale or Participants Factor Treatment Control Total Subscales SE 0.771 0.609 0.743 OES 0.753 0.608 0.722 PES 0.456 0.842 0.583 LG 0.478 0.660 0.596 PGD 0.585 0.491 0.834 PGE 0.863 0.720 0.562 IMP 0.421 0.623 0.480 SCH 0.733 0.745 0.652 VAL 0.685 0.819 0.824 Schema LV 0.586 0.832 0.785 IMC 0.746 0.833 0.736 PGD 0.676 0.746 0.696 PGE 0.822 0.905 0.846 IT 0.421 0.623 0.480 ** p < .01 *** p < .001 Note 1. The schema items listed are LV = Learning Value; IMC = Internal Motivational Controls; PGD = Performance Goal/Self Defeating; PGE = Performance Goal/Self Enhancing; lT = Implicit Theory. 128 Learning Value (LV). Factor 2 consists of items representing three motivational resources (optimistic explanatory style, self efficacy, and schematicity) which appear to represent parent’s internal resources and competency. This more autonomous, independent, self-related, ownership taking factor can be labelled, lntemal Motivational Controls (lMCs). Factor 3 consists only of items pertaining to performance goals including 4 self defeating and 1 self enhancing items. These items appear to represent resources external to parent’s self resources and influenced by others. This more relational, social dependent, attributing to others, less controllable factor can continue to be labelled by its original construct, Performance Goals— Self Defeating (PGD). Factor 4 consists of only self enhancing performance goals and is also labelled by its originally construct, Performance Goals- Self Enhancing (PGE). Factor 5 consists of all of the Implicit Theory (IT) items which directly reflect incremental and entity theorist perspectives. The test-retest reliabilities are generally adequate to excellent and are most stable for the control group participants. The difference in the pre- and post- measures across groups may not necessarily reflect unreliability of the instrument but rather a change in the parent or the parent’s situation. It is possible that some incidental motivational change may have been occurring in the treatment group parents and contributed to the weaker correlations from pre- to post- PMT in comparison to the control group parents. 129 The pilot study addressed the question as to whether the motivational resources formed identifiable motivational schema. These schema are grouped together to form five central schemas (LV, IMC, PGD, PGE, IT). Whether these motivational factors predict outcomes in PMT will be examined in the main study to address the following questions: 1. Do the traditionally examined predictors (severity of child behavior, psychosocial stressors, parent depression, socio-economic status, and single parent families) predict outcomes in PMT(child behavior, parent satisfaction, parent involvement)? 2. Are the individual motivational inner resources and value aspects associated with outcomes of parent management training? Specifically, questions will be asked as to whether these motivational schemas are predictive of various outcomes in parent training? 3. Do the motivational resources change over time and are they susceptible to greater change as a result of participation in a predominantly behaviorally oriented PMT? Hvootheses: The following specific hypotheses are offered in the main study: 1. Parents participating in PMT are likely to demonstrate better outcomes (child behavior change) in comparison to the parents in the wait list control group. a) parents in the treatment group will show a larger drop in ratings of total behavior problems; b) parents in the treatment group will show a larger drop in ratings of externalizing behavior problems; c) parents in both groups will show no change on ratings of internalizing problems. . Predictors of PMT will predict outcomes regarding child behavior change and parent satisfaction as follows: a) The traditionally examined predictors will continue to be predictive of outcomes. b) The motivational factors will emerge among individual parents and be predictive of outcomes. c) A combination of traditionally examined predictors and motivational predictors will be the most predictive of outcomes in parent training. 130 3. 4. iii) Predictors of PMT will predict outcomes regarding parent engagement, i.e. drop out rates: a) The traditionally examined predictors will continue to be predictive of parent drop out outcomes. b) The motivational factors will emerge among individual parents and be predictive of drop out outcomes. c) A combination of traditionally examined predictors and motivational predictors will be the most predictive of drop out outcomes in PMT. The individual motivation resources will also be predictive of outcomes as follows: i) Parent self efficacy for parent behavior management will influence PMT outcomes: a) Parents who are high in self-efficacy will likely use more cognitive strategies, persist longer, and be more consistent than parents who are not high in self efficacy. Thus, higher levels of parent self-efficacy will be associated with sustained and engaged parent involvement. b) Parents with low self efficacy in the behavior management domain will be more likely to drop out of parent training. Parents’ explanatory styles defined as pessimistic or optimistic will be related to drop out outcomes in PMT. a) Parents with optimistic explanatory styles will show greater levels of parent engagement and complete the PMT program. b) Parents with pessimistic explanatory styles will show lower levels of parent engagement and drop out from the PMT program. Parents’ goal orientation will influence drop out outcomes in parent training. a) Parents who demonstrate a performance goal orientation will likely focus on the adequacy of their ability, leaving them vulnerable to the helplessness response, negative affect, setting up impaired performance and translating into ultimately less positive outcomes in PMT than parents with a Ieaming gOal orientation. b) Parents who demonstrate a learning goal orientation will likely focus on increasing their ability over time, predisposing them to effective strategy formation, positive affect, and sustained performance. Thus, these parents will demonstrate more positive outcomes in parent involvement. Parents” implicit theorist orientation is likely to be associated with drop out outcomes in PMT. 131 vi) vii) a) Parents who score higher on the entity theorist scale are more likely to believe in a fixed social-moral reality, invest in the status quo, use more punishment and less positives, and thus, demonstrate little change from pre-, post- testing in terms of parent outcomes and are more likely to drop out. Parents who are schematic in parent training are more likely than aschematic parents to have well-developed possible selves which serve to energize and motivate strategic use of skills and abilities. Consequently, higher schematicity scores will be associated with more positive drop out outcomes. Parents who demonstrate more valuing aspects of motivation (interest, appreciation) within the PMT sessions are more likely to be engaged in PMT group activities, complete homework assignments, and transfer learning to home situations. a) Higher value, interest, and appreciation aspects of motivation will be associated with more positive outcomes on measures of parent involvement/engagement. The expectancy x value model of motivation provides the framework for integrating parent motivation factors relevant to PMT programs. Specifically, relationships are likely between the following; a) the interaction between pessimistic explanatory style and value will predict parent drop out; b) the interaction between optimistic explanatory style and value will predict parent drop out. 5. Parents’ orientations will be associated with drop out as follows: a) Parents who are schematic are less likely to drop out of PMT; b) Parents who are aschematic are more likely to drop out of PMT; 0) Parents with an entity theorist orientation will be associated with parent dropout. d) Parents with an incremental theorist orientation will be associated with parent completion of PMT. 6. As a result of participating in PMT, parents motivational schema will change. a) Parents in the treatment group are likely to show higher post test scores on self-efficacy in comparison to parents in the control group. b) Parents in the treatment group are likely to show more changes on other inner motivational resources in comparison to parents in the control group. c) Parents in the treatment group will show less stability among motivational aspects/resources. 132 Chapter 3 METHODOLOGY Participants Participants are parents of clinic referred children who present as requiring parent management training. The majority of referrals were made by physicians (general practitioners and pediatricians) in a health care organization. Analysis of the history of physician referrals indicates few referrals of children demonstrating “normal” patterns. In the past 2 years of physician referrals for various learning, behavior, and language disorders, 98 % were assessed to have symptoms within clinical ranges. Other participants were referred by psychological and social work staff from the local ADHD clinic and self initiated contact by phoning for an orientation session. As a result, it was impossible to determine the number of parents who had initially been referred. Of the parents who attended orientation sessions and initial assessment sessions, six of the 99 refused to participate in the research study. Ninety-three parents with children who range in age from 4 ‘/2 to 12 years old (at first contact) served as participants. Each parent had children enrolled in a general education, preschool, junior kindergarten to grade 8 classroom and whom the parent described as demonstrating disruptive behavior problems. Forty-six children met DSM-IV diagnostic criteria for a disruptive behavior disorder, 35 were diagnosed with a comorbid diagnoses, 7 diagnosed with another disorder, and 2 with no diagnosis 133 (Table 24). Detailed demographic information divided according to treatment and control groups is provided in Tables 24 and 25. Frequency tables for both parent and child characteristics by group were calculated. Descriptives were calculated for various other identifying characteristics. All parents have agreed and signed a consent form (see Appendix B for Consent Form for Participation in Research Project). Procedures Procedures followed three primary phases: 1) orientation interview and baseline assessment; 2) intervention; and 3) a one-month follow-up. During Phase 1, participants came into the clinic for an initial orientation/assessment individual session. At the orientation session, the intervention study was explained to parents and they were asked if they would like to be involved. Prior to beginning the study portion of the interview, parents received information about the study and were asked to sign a consent for research form (see Appendix B). Contingent upon their written consent, parents then completed independent additional questionnaires. Although their interview and/or assessment results were not used in the data analyses, any parents who did not consent were not excluded from participating in the PMT sessions. The individual interviews and questionnaire completions took approximately 1 to 1 1/2 hours to complete in the clinic. Some of the parents brought the questionnaires home and either mailed them back or returned them at the first session. As a result of this process, 8 questionnaires and survey forms were not returned. 134 Table 24 Demgqraphic Characteristics of Treatment and Control ParticipJants in PMT Study (n = 93) Participants Descriptive Treatment Control Total Q 0/o fl 0/0 fl 0/0 Parent gender Male 17 26.6 7 24.1 24 25.5 Female 47 73.4 22 75.9 69 73.4 Parent education Elementary 1 1.6 2 6.9 3 3.2 High School 19 29.7 13 44.8 32 34.0 College (Community) 28 43.8 10 34.5 38 40.4 University (undergraduate) 10 15.6 3 10.3 13 13.8 University (graduate) 5 7.8 1 3.4 5 5.3 Ethnicity/ Race Native Canadian or Inuit 13 20.3 2 6.9 15 16.0 White / Caucasian 50 78.1 26 89.7 76 80.9 Current Annual Household Income ($) Under 10,000 8 12.5 2 6.9 10 3.4 10,000 — 14,999 13 20.3 5 17.2 18 6.1 15,000 - 19,999 5 7.8 5 17.2 10 3.4 20,000 - 24,999 8 12.5 2 6.9 10 3.4 25,000 — 29,999 0 0 1 3.4 1 0.3 30,000 — 34,999 0 0 1 3.4 1 0.3 35,000 - 39,999 2 3.1 4 13.8 6 2.0 40,000 — 44,999 2 3.1 1 3.4 3 1.0 45,000 -- 49,999 6 9.4 1 3.4 7 2.4 50,000 — 54,999 5 7.8 3 10.3 8 2.7 55,000 — 59,999 4 6.3 0 0 4 1.4 60,000 — 69,999 4 6.3 2 6.9 6 2.0 70,000 and over 7 10.9 1 3.4 8 2.7 Low Income Cutoffs (Stats Canada) Low 25 39.1 12 41.4 37 12.6 High 39 60.9 16 55.2 56 19.0 Single Parent Household No (married, common law, etc.) 39 60.9 19 65.5 58 19.7 I Yes (separated, divorced, widowed) 25 39.1 9 31 34 11.6 Note. Totals do not sum to 100% due to missing data. a n=64 for treatment group b n=29 for wait list control c n=93 for total 135 Table 25 Demographic Characteristics of Children Whose PLmnts Participated in the PMT Study (n=93) Participants Treatment Totals Characteristic % % % Child Gender Male 42 65.6 72.4 63 67 Female 22 34.4 24.1 30 30.9 Child Education Preschool 1 1.6 1 3.4 2 2.1 Junior Kindergarten 8 12.5 2 6.9 10 10.6 Senior Kindergarten 3 4.7 0 0 3 3.2 Grade 1 13 20.3 2 6.9 15 16 Grade 2 4 6.3 2 6.9 6 6.4 Grade 3 10 15.6 4 13.8 14 14.9 Grade 4 9 14.1 3 10.3 12 12.8 Grade 5 6 9.4 5 17.2 11 11.7 Grade 6 7 10.9 7 24.1 14 14.9 Grade 7 1 1.6 2 6.7 3 3.2 Grade 8 1 1.6 0 0 1 1.1 Special Education 1 1.6 1 1.6 2 2.1 Child Medication None 34 53.1 22 75.9 56 59.6 Ritalin 26 40.6 4 13.8 30 31.9 Other Psychotropic 4 6.3 2 6.9 6 6.4 Child Diagnosis None 2 3.1 0 0 2 2.1 ADHD 7 10.9 3 10.3 10 10.6 DBD-NOS 28 43.8 8 27.6 36 38.3 ADHD Comorbid with 23 35.9 2 41.4 35 37.2 Other DBD Other 3 4.7 4 13.8 7 7.5 Previous Treatments None 14 21.9 4 13.8 18 19.1 Psychotherapy/Counselling 13 20.3 2 6.8 15 16.8 Leaming/Resource 14 21.9 9 31 23 24.5 Combined 22 34.4 2 41.4 34 37.8 Note. Values do not sum to 100% due to missing data. 136 Length of time between orientation interview and the first session varied because of this process. Parents were initially randomly assigned to a treatment or wait list control group. Despite random assignment, the treatment and control groups differed in terms of specific characteristics (e.g., Ritalin use and previous psychotherapy treatment). During Phase 2 (Intervention), participants in the PMT Treatment groups did not receive a reminder phone call about the intervention sessions. Intervention closely followed Barkley’s (1997) training procedures in four, two- hour, weekly sessions and a one month follow up meeting, totaling 8 weeks. During Phase 3 (Follow-up), PMT Treatment parents participated in a follow up booster session 1 month after termination of the initial parent management training. All treatment group participants (PMT and Control Participants) participated in post-PMT interviews 1 month after initial treatment. Control Group participants were offered parent management training sessions at this point. Settinq and Materials An out-patient health care organization, The Group Health Centre (GHC), provided the setting and participants for the proposed study. An initial formal request was made to the Department Head of Quality Assurance who scheduled a proposal presentation to the Research and Ethics Review Board of the organization (see Appendix B for Letter Requesting Permission and Algorithm of Research and Ethics Review Committee Process). 137 All parent interview and training sessions took place in the same office/conference room locations throughout the same outpatient health care organization. Individual orientation interviews and assessments were conducted in an office in the Communication Disorders Department, located one building west of the Main Building. This corner office is extremely large and comfortable, next to the Psychiatry Department. When appointments overlapped and parents required extra time to complete independent questionnaires, parents were moved to one of the adjacent offices, depending upon availability. Training sessions were conducted in the Women's Health Centre Conference Room, which seats 20 people and is located in a third building just south of the Main Building. Participants had no difficulty locating free parking in the large lots at each of these clinic locations. Non-clinic members were charged a token fee (to cover the cost of photocopying and other materials). Othenrvise, the individual consult/orientation and subsequent training sessions were provided without charging a fee for service. Sessions were offered in the mornings, afternoons, and evenings to accommodate parents schedules and babysitting constraints. Parents were allowed to move among groups in order to facilitate attendance, especially since several parents were employed in shift work schedules. In this fashion, several attempts were made to remove some of the barriers to treatment traditionally discussed in the PMT literature. The Parent Management training followed the step by step procedures as detailed in Defiant chiIJdren: A clinicfli’s manual for assessment a_nd parent 138 training (Barkley, 1997). Nine of the ten steps1 were covered in two-hour sessions as outlined in Table 26. Parents were provided with the detailed manual handouts which summarized each of the steps covered in training as well as corresponding homework assignments, developed by the therapist. Procedures for parent training explicitly followed the parent training manual instructions with the following exceptions to treatment manual protocol: 1) Steps were combined in order that the parent training sessions were completed within two months rather than in ten plus weekly sessions; 2) Steps 3, 4, and 5 were supplemented with materials from other parent training manuals and programs (Alvord, 1973; Greenberg & Horn, 1991; Wells, 1993); 3) Step 8 was omitted, as noted previously; 4) Coffee breaks and refreshments were provided in between each step; 5) The majority of parents participated in small group instruction (6-8 parents) as suggested in the manual but group size varied as some were larger (10-12 parents) and some were smaller (3-4 parents) depending upon attendance dates. 6) Parents were required to practice newly learned skills using standard homework assignments which were collected in subsequent weeks. Parents assigned to the Control group did not participate in parent training until follow up data had been gathered (1 month post-PMT). The particular parent training approaches/procedures to be utilized varied and were somewhat dependent upon the present study results. Data Collection A battery of self report questionnaires was used to study the constructs believed to be related to PMT. Data were gathered at Baseline (1-2 weeks prior 1 Step 8: Improving School Behavior from Home: The Daily School Behavior Report Card was omitted as the study data gathering was also conducted during summer months. 139 000005. 00-26.60 £005. 000 000 00.0000 00080 x 0 050.090 0030000 0550 05.0ch x. 0000.0 0.500 0. 000.20 00.00005. H0E0.0o.0 00.00.0000. .5 v 532802 050 9 so ms; 0:053 _> 000505. 000.3005 .050 00< 0:0 0.0.... > 0 000.00 000 00.00 03.00 0.00000 62 0. 00.0.0 :0:>> >_ 50.0 0000000000. 000 0000._0E00 00.000000. :. N 03000.0 00 .. 9,280.2 09220 0.2. _ F 2E 090 5.82, A500F $00000. 00.0.0.0 0.0.-.00 0. 02.000 00 00500090 0000 >0 d000 0N 0.00.0 140 to training), Pre-PMT (at the first PMT session), Post-PMT (at the last PMT session), and Post-PMT (1 month after training) as indicated. Traditional/v Examined Predictors: Information regarding traditionally examined predictors was collected at baseline session as part of the evaluation process for the initial intake interview and assessment. Parents completed a general information sheet to assess marital status, socio-economic status (SES), and number of psychosocial stressor predictors. Marital status was coded as 0 for single parent households and 1 for two parent households. Hollingshead and Redlich’s two factor index (1958), which utilizes educational and occupational attainment in its formula, was used to reflect socioeconomic status level (SES). Socioeconomic status was also designated as low income or high income based on a formula published by Statistics Canada (1998). This latter formula is current and utilizes various factors (household income and number of members supported by this income) to estimate low income cutoffs (LICO). Thus, SES as a predictor was used as a dichotomous (LICO) and continuous variable (Hollingshead 2 Factor Index) due to the very different nature of both measures (see Appendix E for Methods of Calculating SES) Parent depression was derived through total scores on the Beck Depression Inventory -2"d Edition (BDl-ll). The BDI-II is a self-report scale of 21 items assessing level of depression in clinical and normal participants. Each item contains 4 statements about a particular symptom of depression and are 141 arranged in increasing severity. Parents select the statement that best describes their affect over the previous two week span. lntemal consistency of the BDI-ll is high as the coefficient alpha for outpatients and students was .92 and .93, respectively (Beck, Steer, & Brown, 1996). Evidence of the convergent and discriminant validity of the BDI-ll indicates a positive correlation (.71) with depression on the Revised Hamilton Psychiatric Rating Scale for Depression and robust discriminant validity between depression and anxiety (.47) using the Revised Hamilton Anxiety Scale (Beck et al., 1996). Severity of child behavior was derived through parent responses on the Home Situations Questionnaire (HSQ). The HSQ is a rating scale which provides a measure of child behavior in 14 contexts, specific to home and public settings. Behaviors are rated according to whether children have problems in any of those situations and according to how severe on a scale of 1 to 9 (where 1-miId, 9- severe). A total number count and mean severity score is calculated. Gender and age based norms are available for this scale and can be used to interpret the individual child’s severity of behavior in comparison to peers. The HSQ is reported to have satisfactory test-retest reliability and to correlate significantly with other parent completed rating scales (Barkley, 1990). Information from the interview and questionnaires was used to gather demographic information and summarized in the Participant Section as well as in table format (T ables 24 and 25). Data were gathered from each individual participant (including both parents if involved) for each predictor and subsequently analyzed. 142 Parent Motivational Aspects/ Schema No published measures were found in the literature regarding parent motivation and PMT. Thus, the PMSS was developed specifically for this project. Parent information on motivational aspects was gathered through the administration of the PMSS, which is an adaptation of 44 domain specific questionnaire items taken from the research literature on motivation (see Appendix A for Parent Motivation Schema Scale). The content validity of the PMSS was attempted through inter-rater judgements and a factor analysis procedure on the pilot sample. A content validity index was computed using the approach by Waltz, Strickland, and Lenz (1991). Based on the raters’ judgements, the original 55 items were maintained, revised, or deleted from the scale. The resulting 44 items in the scale were pilot tested on 200 parents of children attending a health care organization, community agency, and/or school system. Support for the reliability of the PMSS was demonstrated through internal consistency via Cronbach’s alpha (on both the pilot and study samples) and test-retest reliability (on the study groups i.e., treatment and control groups). This scale was administered both pre- and post- PMT. Parents were asked to rate their motivation in two domain specific areas: a) regarding participation in the PMT intervention; and, b) ' motivational aspects concerning their child’s disruptive behavior on a 5- point scale from not true of me at all (1) to extremely true of me (5). Individual items of the PMSS which reflect nine motivational aspects, were grouped into nine subscales, respectively. The self efficacy subscale 143 assesses parent concern with how well or poorly people judge or believe that they will perform on a task give their skills and a variety of circumstances (Bandura, 1997, p.37). A sample item from this subscale is “If my child became disruptive and noisy in public places (malls, church), I feel assured that I know some techniques to redirect him or her quickly. “ The optimistic explanatory style and pessimistic explanatory style subscales assess attributional aspects of motivation. For example, an optimistic explanatory style involves attributing child misbehavior to 1) external, 2) unstable, 3) controllable dimensions while a pessimistic explanatory style involves attributing child misbehavior to 1) lntemal, 2) stable, and 3) uncontrollable dimensions. Sample items from these subscales are, “when my child is in one of his/her moods, there is usually something that set him off but can be handled" and “ my child’s behavior is mostly biological, a big part of his/her personality, and sometimes, we ’re lucky to get through one of his/her temper tantrums”, respectively. The Ieaming goals subscale assesses parent approach to PMT and child management challenges as the desire to increase competence and improve skill. A sample item from this scale includes, ”I like parent training work I’ll Ieam from even ifl make a lot of mistakes.” The performance goals-self enhancing subscale assesses the levels parents strive to demonstrate superior abilities, do better than others, or outperform others (Skaalvik, 1997). A sample item is, “I would like parent training activities that let me show how good lam at managing behavior”. The performance 144 goals-self defeating subscale assesses the level parents strive to avoid looking stupid and being negatively judged by others (Skaalvik, 1997). A sample here involves, “When interacting with my child in public places, I am concerned not to make a fool of myself. A short subscale assessed whether parents were entity or incremental theorists according to implicit theories. According to implicit theory definitions (Brophy, 1998; Hong et al., 1997; Levy et al., 1998), parents who are entity theorists view their child’s behavior as a fixed entity which cannot be changed and over which they have no control. Those parents who are incremental theorists view their child’s behavior as responsive to their management effort . All of the sample items, measure level of entity theory orientation, with low scores implying parents may hold an incremental theory. A sample item illustrates this point: “my child’s behavior is something very basic about him/her and it can‘t be changed much.” Parents with scores of 7 or higher were deemed entity theorists while parents with scores less than 7 were deemed incremental theorists. An even shorter subscale assesses parent schematicity which involves parent recognition that they have a particular ability in handling child behavior and belief that this ability is important (Cross & Markus, 1994). The items used to identify schematic and aschematic parents were summed to arrive at a total schematicity/ self schema score. For further analysis of parent engagement measured by drop-out or program completion, parents with scores of 5 or above on these items were 145 classified as schematic. Parents with scores of less than 5 were classified as aschematic. All other parents who do not clearly fall into schematic or aschematic categories were not selected at this level of analysis. Several items comprise the value subscale which is a relatively new aspect and consideration in motivational research. A sample item of this scale is, “involvement in PMT will be helpful in improving the quality of our lives.” Each of these subscale items is summed to be used in select data analysis. In addition, the implicit theory and schematicity items are converted into categorical variables as described above. A principal component analysis of the PMSS yielded five factors. Thus, the nine subscales were not supported by the factor analysis. When the five factor solution was produced, the orthogonal factors were labelled according to their respective factor loadings. Factor 1 was labelled Learning Value because it consisted of only Ieaming goals and value items. Factor 2 was labelled lntemal Motivational Controls because it consisted of self efficacy, optimistic explanatory style, and schematicity items. Factors 3, 4, and 5 consisted of the original motivational aspects and retained the names of Performance Goals-Self Defeating, Performance Goals-Self Enhancing, and Implicit Theory. Outcomes The following measures are organized according to PMT outcome categories. 146 Child Behavior Changes Pre—, Post-, and Long-term outcome measures regarding changes in child behavior were based on information from the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL/4- 18) (Achenbach, 1991). The CBCL data were obtained from one parent of each child participant. Parents rate their child’s behavior on 118 problem behavior items where O = not true as far as you know, 1 = somewhat or sometimes true, 2: very true or often true. The CBCU 4-18 provides raw scores which are converted to normalized T-scores and percentiles grouped on three scales (Total Problems, Internalizing, and Externalizing) and 8 syndrome scales. Reliability and validity data are well established and detailed in the manual (Achenbach, 1991). For instance, reliability information from the manual indicates that test- retest coefficients for the Behavior Problem scales are high (.95) and also high for the Social Competence Scale (.99) (Achenbach, 1991). Child behavior change scores were calculated by subtracting post-CBCL t-scores from pre- CBCL t-scores for each respective scale (Total, lntemalizing, and Externalizing Problems). Long term child behavior change scores were calculated by subtracting long-term CBCL t-scores from pre-CBCL scores. Parent Satisfaction Post- outcome measures regarding parent satisfaction were based on information from the Therapy Attitude Inventory (T Al)(Eyberg, 1993). The TAI is a brief consumer satisfaction measure of parent training, parent-child treatments, and family therapy with established psychometric evidence of reliability and validity (Brestan et al., 1999). Support for internal consistency of the TAI was 147 provided by Cronbach’s alpha coefficients of .91 (excellent) and a stability coefficient over 4-months of .85 (high). Evidence of external validity (construct validity) was demonstrated through positive correlations (.36 to .49) between scores on the TAI and a) the Eyberg Child Behavior Inventory, and b) observations of behavioral compliance (Brestan et al., 1999). A principal component analysis on the TAI items yields two factors: 1) a satisfaction with outcome factor which represents parent satisfaction with child behavior changes and 2) a satisfaction with process factor which represents parent satisfaction with the components of treatment, including parenting skills taught and general approach to treatment (Table 27) (Brestan et al., 1999). The 10 items of the TAI reflect satisfaction with parenting skills learned, child behavior changes, and the type of program used, which are all derived to reflect goals of parent-child treatments (Brestan et al., 1999). Parents rate items on a scale from 1 (no satisfaction) to 5 (maximum satisfaction) and yield a total score between 10 and 50. Parent Engagement A final outcome measure involved estimates of parent engagement and parent disengagement. Information for parent engagement characterization was based upon an exposure measure 1) number of hours attended in all sessions; 2) number of homework assignments completed, and 3) number of participants who dropped out and did not complete the program. The latter was further differentiated according to point of drop off as pre-PMT, pari-PMT, post-PMT, long term-PMT. Given the smaller sample size and lower statistical power, 148 Table 27 Factor loadinfion the Therapy Attitude Inventory (TAI). Item Content Factor 1 Factor 2 1. Learned useful techniques for discipling 0.39 0.72 2. Learned useful techniques for teaching 0.11 0.87 3. Status of relationship with child 0.81 0.34 4. Ability to discipline child 0.80 0.32 5. Major behavior problems of child 0.82 0.33 6. Child's noncompliance to commands 0.85 0.29 7. General progress of child 0.83 0.33 8. Problems unrelated to child 0.31 0.69 9. Type program used to improve child behavior 0. 72 0.15 10. General feeling about the program 0.39 0.57 Note. Numbers in italic represent strongest factor loadings for each item. (adapted from Eyberg & Brestan, 1999) 149 engagement was analyzed by dropout while attendance and homework completion measures were summarized in demographic information. Desiqn and Analysis The present study was a longitudinal design consisting of three types of variables: traditionally examined predictors, motivational predictors, and outcomes in PMT. The focus of the present longitudinal study was to examine the relationship among traditional predictors (single parenthood, severity of child behavior, parent depression, SES, and number of psychosocial stressors) and potential novel predictors (parent motivation aspects) on three categories of outcomes in parent training (changes in child behavior, parent satisfaction, and parent engagement). Descriptive statistics and displays were calculated before formal analyses. Initially, several analyses comparing the treatment and control groups were performed using the individual CBCL pre- and post- t-scores for each scale as well as behavior change scores, in order to establish any treatment effects. Several other analyses were performed to test the several hypotheses of the current study (Table 28). Analysis I: Treatment Effects To test the hypothesis that parents in PMT would demonstrate better outcomes, (greater child behavior change) in comparison to parents in the wait list control group, the first analysis consisted of a 2 X 2 X 3 factorial design with repeated measures. The 2 groups (treatment and control) by the two time points (pre- and post-) by the 3 CBCL Scales 150 Table 28 Summa_rv_of Specific Hypotheses (abbreviated). Measuresl and Analyses Measure Hypothesis Abbreviated Description Pre- Post- Analysis I Treatment effects: CBCL CBCL 2'3'3 Factorial Treatment versus control parents Anova II Continuous outcome variables CBCL CBCL' will be predicted by continuous predictors TAl TAI a. TEP predict child behavior HSQ canonical change and parent satisfaction Stress Checklist correlation BDl-ll Hollingshead Index b. Motivation schema predict PMSS canonical child behavior and parent satisfaction correlation c. combination of both types of same as a. & b. canonical predictors predict child behavior above correlation and parent satisfaction lll Discrete outcome variable (drop out) count of treatment count of treatment will be predicted by continuous predictors participants participants a. TEP predict drop out HSQ discriminant Stress Checklist function BDl-ll LICO SPF b. Motivation schema predict PMSS discriminant parent drop out factor totals function c. combination of both types of same as a. 8. b. discriminant predictors predict drop out above function W Individual motivational resources predict PMSS logistic drop out subscales regression count of treatment count of treatment participants participants V Parents' theoretical orientations are PMSS chi-square associated with drop out schematicity subscale analysis implicit theory subscale count of treatment count of treatment participants participants Vl Parents who participate in PMT experience PMSS PMSS pair-wise t-test a change in motivational schema and/or factor totals factor totals subscales subscales subscales 151 (Total, Internalizing, Externalizing) were compared in a two way analysis of variance. For the purpose of brevity, all tests for normality and equality of variance were met unless otherwise specified in the results. The appropriate post hoc analyses (A Bonferroni post hoc test) was conducted based on an assumption of equal variances. Analysis ll: Linear Relations Among Continuous Variables To test the hypotheses that traditional predictors and motivational predictors would predict child behavior change and parent satisfaction with PMT, a canonical correlation was conducted in the second analysis. The canonical correlation examined the linear relationship among the continuous predictor variables both traditional (severity of child behavior, parent depression, number of stressors, SES) and motivational ( the five factors) with the linear relationship among the continuous outcome variables (changes in child behavior and parent satisfaction). The appropriateness of canonical correlation techniques is well established given the multiple predictors and multiple outcomes. Basic tests of significance associated with the canonical correlation were considered and two common tools for interpretations were provided. While interpretation of loadings or standardized coefficients is the most widely used , primarily for reasons of multicolinearity among variables within sets (Levine, 1977; Thompson, 1984), standardized coefficients are described as the most useful (Rencher, 1995). The standardized coefficients remove the effects of scaling as well as provide the most information regarding the 152 importance of a given variable in the context of the others (Rencher, 1995). Analysis Ill: Classifying According to Drop Out The third analysis was conducted to test the hypothesis that traditional predictors and motivational predicts would be associated with parent engagement (i.e., drop out of PMT). This third statistical analysis employed a discriminant function analysis to evaluate whether the traditional predictors and/or motivational predictors were useful to screen or classify participants known to drop out of PMT (parent engagement outcome). Tests for the equality of variance, independence, and linearity were met unless othenrvise specified. The two multivariate techniques above (canonical and discriminant) were conducted to evaluate the relationships among the predictors and outcomes in PMT. As a reflection of the mutual influence of the variables on each other, it was assumed that statistical results would change if some variables were added or deleted. Interest in the current study was centered on the joint performance of the traditional and new motivational predictor variables at hand. Mechanisms were in place to conduct an evaluation and comparison involving a traditional model and new model of predictors in parent training. Thus, the two categories of predictor variables were added to each of the statistical analyses in three stages or models. 153 First, each multivariate statistical analyses included an analysis with the traditional predictors in the model. Second, statistical analysis used the motivational predictors in the new model. This indicated the maximum possible change on outcomes by manipulating new predictors. Third, each statistical procedure used both the traditional and motivational predictors in a combined model which indicated a more realistic appraisal of possible change on outcomes. These three models (traditional, motivational, and combined) were compared for each multivariate statistical procedure to test the third, fourth, and fifth hypotheses (Table 28). Analysis IV: Using Motivational Subscales to Classify According to Dron Out The fourth analysis tested the hypothesis that individual motivational resources would be predictive of PMT outcomes. This fourth statistical analysis employed a logistic regression running the discrete category outcome variable (drop out versus completers) with the individual motivational resources (see Appendix D for Rationale for Using Subscales). Logistic regression was included because it 1) requires far fewer assumptions than discriminant analysis; and 2) to specifically evaluates for any interaction effects among the motivational predictors. Analysis V: Univariate Examination of Discrete Variables The fifth analysis tested the hypothesis that parent orientation (schematic versus aschematic and entity versus incremental) would be associated with drop out. A series of chi-square analyses were also 154 conducted among the drop out outcome variable of the PMT study and the parents’ schematicity and implicit theory orientations, respectively, and among other categorical traditional predictors (single parenthood, low income cutoff). As well, analyses were also conducted for drop out and other parent or child descriptors. The Chi-Square Two Way Test for Association was computed to determine whether or not any of the two paired variables were independent of each other (Horvath, 1981 ). Analysis Vl: Change in Parents’ Motivational Schema Finally, the sixth analysis tested the hypothesis that parents’ motivational schema and individual resources would show greater change for the treatment group participants than for the control group participants. A series of t-tests were conducted to test this hypothesis. Table 28 summarizes the specific hypotheses (abbreviated) with measures used and analyses employed. An alpha level of .05 was used for all statistical tests. A Bonferroni adjustment was considered and employed when multiple statistical tests were conducted. 155 Chapter 4 RESULTS Analysis I: Treatment Effects The results of the descriptive information and Factorial Anova test the hypothesis that treatment group parents would demonstrate better outcomes. The mean CBCL scores for each group (treatment and control), each time point (pre-, post-, long term-), and each scale (Total, lntemalizing, and Externalizing problems) are summarized in Table 29. T-scores greater than 65 are indicative of clinically significant behavior problems. Treatment group behavior scales indicate a decline in average behavior problems over pre-, post— , and long term- PMT for all scales. In fact, behavior for the treatment group participants had improved from clinically significant ranges at baseline (t-score >= 65) to behavior within normal limits (t-score <60) in comparison to other children their age at one month follow up. Control group scales indicate an increase in average behavior problems over pre- and post- time periods (Figure 2). Visual inspection of the CBCL data also suggests a slight decrease in median behavior problems for the treatment group and a slight increase for the control group, across all scales (Figure 3). Within scales, the treatment group results indicate a larger decline in total problems, smaller decline in internalizing problems, and little, if any change in externalizing problems (Figures 4, 5, and 6). The control group results indicate a 156 .00 .0 .00.0000 000.090. 0:90 .908 .9 0N.0 0.0900 .00" .E0-0oEF 000 .08 0......) 0.0 n 0590 E00009. 0.0 .9 0N.0 0.0E0m .N 0.02 .25.. 0050009 00.00.0000 0009.00: 000 3000. .o.>0...00 0..:0 00 0.500. 000. 000. .0. 0:90 .958 .0.. :03 00 000.98 .00 0.03 0.00 .F 0.02 157 I...- ll... 00. F no.9 N_<... ll.- --- 5.0 00.00 :5. it: it: 006 00.00 .05-F 0000020050 02.00.0000 .00.00 it: .1..- 0.0.0 F $.00 -_..>_0-0oE F mm. F F 00.3. 00.0 $.00 ..20-0o0 «v. F F .00.00 00.3 3.00 0.20-0.0 00_N..00.0.0. - 000 E... .1..- 3:0. F 00.00 0.5.0-000. F 00. F F 00.00 00.3 00.3. 0.5.0-000 00.0 F 00.00 00. F F R00 0.20-0.0 9..~._00.0Fxm. - 000 l...- ------ 00. F F 00.00 0.5.0-000. F 00.3 00.00 F00 F000 .:20-00o0 mod F 00.00 00.0 00.00 0.20-0.0 00.0390 .05-F - 000 60. z 60. z .9500 E00000... 05000.2 0.00050 0000.00.00 00.00..0> 00.0050 0:50.000 5 000.0300 90000.0. 00.000 0000.00.00 000.). 0N 0.000. T—score 80< 50— TIME 40- N= H 786’ fii foe " f 34 60 Treatment Control Figure 2. Pre- Post— Scores of CBCL Results 158 T-score . ‘Group 40: ‘ ‘ .Treatment 304‘ ,, -_ fl ,, --- -. -,. fir 7 7 - ‘ 2423.35“ Control N= 98 4e 98 48 98 48 Total lntemalizing Externalizing Figure 3. CBCL Results Across All Scales 159 T-score 80f ‘ 70- 50< N= ae'HT ’36" if” 26 ' 56' Treatment Control Figure 4. CBCL Total Subscale Results 160 Total Subscale l ‘ - Pre- Post- T-score 70 — 60 4 50 - 1 ‘ Internalizing Scale " l 40 . - ‘ T ’ l - Pre- l 7 l __ 30_l,,.__,,-_1- .- _ fig? - - 2:3“ Post- N = 36 36 20 20 Treatment Control Figure 5. CBCL Internalizing Subscale Results 161 T-score 50- l Externalizing Scale 40- r ' , , -_ ‘ - Pre- l - 30.- 77 77 7 77 7 7 -Post- N = 36 36 20 20 Treatment Control Figure 6. CBCL Externalizing Subscale Results 162 slight decline in total problems, increase in internalizing problems, and slight decrease in externalizing problems (Figures 4, 5, and 6). Means and standard deviations for each of the behavior change scores are shown in Table 30. From pre— to post- PMT, the treatment group has the largest average change score for total child behavior while the control group has the smallest average change score for externalizing behaviors. The majority of changes from pre— to post— PMT, with the exception of internalizing problems, are in a positive direction suggesting a decline in child behavior problems. The control group had a relative large average change score for internalizing behaviors in a negative direction indicating a perceived increase in these child behaviors. From pre- to long term- PMT, all of the treatment group average change scores are noticeably larger across all scales indicating a decrease in perception of problem behaviors after PMT. The first formal analysis (Factorial Anova with Repeated Measures) indicated a significant main effect for CBCL Scale, F (2, 338 ) =22.071, p <. 001, and for Group * Scale, F (2,338) =7.661, p <.OO1. All other main effects and interactions were not significant (Table 31). A Bonferroni comparison indicated a significant difference between the internalizing and total scales, p < .001, and internalizingand externalizing scales of the CBCL, p < .001. Figures 7 and 8 present graphic representations of the Group * Scale interaction. From visual inspection, it appears that the mean CBCL t-score relates not only to the particular scale and to the group of the participants, but also a combination of the values of those variables. Treatment group participants rate total and 163 Table 30 Mean Change Scores (Standard Deviations) from CBCL Measures Participants CBCL Scale Treatment (n=37) Control (n=29) M L_lsd M £011 Pre minus Post Total 1 .50 5.44 0.45 5.92 Internalizing 0.92 7.16 -1.45 5.90 Externalizing 0.47 4.89 0.25 8.03 Pre minus Long Term Total 5.58 6.51 ............ Internalizing 4.77 9,13 ............ Externalizing 4.94 6.57 ............ Note. Long term CBCLs for control group were not administered. 164 Table 31 Analysis of Variance Results for Main Effects and Interaction Effects of Group. Time PointLand Scale on Child Behavior Changes (CBCL) Source 0 M§ E g9. Between Subiects Group 1 95.066 0.251 0.617 Error 1 90 378.247 Within Subiects Time (pre- , post-) 1 9.803 0.377 0.540 Scale (total, internal, external) 2 574.419 22.071 0000*" Group * Time 1 28.375 1.090 0.297 Group * Scale 2 199.375 7.661 0.001 *** Time * Scale 2 4.187 0.161 0.851 Group * Time * Scale 2 9.775 0.376 0.687 Error 2 338 26.026 * g < .05. ** g < .01. ***_g < .001 165 T-score l ‘ 1 - Group - Treatment .' - - --- - v" . --. -' .- N = 98 48 98 4a 7 98 48 Total lntemalizing Externalizing Figure 7. Boxplot Depicting Group * Scale Interaction 166 T-score l Group - Treatment 7 Control Total N lntemalizing ”7 Externalizing Figure 8. Bar Graph Depicting Group * Scale Interaction 167 externalizing behaviors with the highest scores. Control group participants rate the total scale with the highest score and rate both internalizing and externalizing problems with equivalent scores. Thus, the results of the first analysis do not support the overall hypothesis that treatment group participates would demonstrate better outcomes (child behavior change) in comparison to control group participants. The treatment effects appear dependent upon CBCL scale and given the long term CBCL treatment group results, may also depend upon time after treatment. Analysis ll: Linear Relations Among Continuous Variables To test the hypothesis that PMT predictors would predict child behavior change and parent satisfaction, the second analysis (canonical correlation) determined a linear relationship between the continuous data set of predictors and outcome variables across the three models. In the traditional predictor model, only the first canonical variate accounted for a significant relationship between the two sets of variables ( r = .816, p < .018). The first variate contained largest coefficients for severity of child behavior as defined by responses on the HSQ and number of psychosocial stressors in set 1 (predictors) and internalizing behavior problems (both post and long term), long term externalizing problems, and parent satisfaction with both process and outcome in set 2 (outcomes) (Table 32). It is noteworthy that maximum loadings are evident in severity of child behavior and parent depression for the first set and all child behavior scores on the second set, highlighting potential differences between interpreting standardized scores versus loadings or structure 168 Table 32 Correlations and Standardized Canonical Coefficients and Their Canonical Variates in Separate Models First Variate Second Variate Canonical Correlation coefficient Canonical Variable Correlation coefficient jaditiorLaL Model One PREDICTORS: HSQ-Severity 0.972 0.995 -0.233 -0.098 Number of Stressors 0.179 -0.254 -0.868 -1.034 Parent Depression 0.468 0.154 0.247 0.567 SES 0.096 0.066 -0.396 0.151 OUTCOMES: Post- CBC-Extemalizing 0.822 0.044 -0.244 0.929 CBC-Internalizing 0.852 0.984 0.184 0.021 Long-Term- CBC-Externalizing 0.706 0.620 -0.551 -1.914 CBC-Internalizing 0.637 -0.662 -0.030 0.827 Parent Satisfaction TAI-1 -0.056 0.457 0.310 0.609 TAI-2 -0.356 -0.380 -0.025 -0.219 Motigtionai Model Two PREDICTORS: 1.Val, LG 0.852 0.752 -0.325 -0.255 2. SE, OES, Sch 0.575 0.354 -0.206 -0.247 3. PGE, PGD -0.293 -0.047 -0.468 -0.292 4. PGE -0.187 -0.238 -0.100 -0.084 5. lmplicit 0.317 0.306 0.889 0.811 OUTCOMES: Post- CBC-Externalizing 0.270 0.41 1 0.254 1.461 CBC-Internalizing -0. 190 -0.477 0.137 -1.077 Long-Term- CBC-Externalizing 0.407 1.020 0.262 -0.825 CBC-Internalizing -0.104 -0.675 0.364 0.630 Parent Satisfaction TAI-1 0.290 0.246 0.406 1.057 TAI-2 0.579 0.418 -0.383 0873 Model One; = .816, p < .018 Model Two 5 = .795, p < .280 169 coefficients. The second variate (r = .608, p < .376) contained largest coefficients on number of stressors and parent depression with externalizing problems (both post and long term), and parent satisfaction (Table 32). In the motivational predictor model, none of the canonical variates accounted for a significant relationship (5 = .795, p < .280). Researchers employ significance tests only as a minimal criteria in deciding which canonical functions to interpret as a result of the large samples required to invoke the central limit theorem and to minimize the tendency of canonical correlation to capitalize on sampling error (Thompson, 1984). Largest coefficients for the first variate included four predictors (factors 1, 2, 5, 4) with five outcomes (post and long term externalizing problems, long term internalizing problems, and parent satisfaction with outcome) (Table 32). The combined model yielded a significant first variate ([ = .950, p < .016) with maximal loadings on five predictors (number of stressors, severity of child behavior, IT, PGD, LV) and maximal loadings on two outcomes (post internalizing problems and satisfaction with process) (Table 33). The second variate ( r_= 813, p <.511) yielded maximal loadings on five predictors (severity of child behavior, stress, parent depression, factor 1, and factor 5) with two outcomes (externalizing and internalizing problems on the long term outcome). In comparing the three models only the traditional model and the combined models had significant canonical variates (Table 34). As SES failed to load even moderately onto any of the previous models, it was removed in a subsequent model leaving the remaining variables. While only 170 Table 33 Correlations and Standardized Canonical Coefficients a_nd Their Canonical Variates i_n_a Combined Model First Variate Second Variate Canonical Canonical Variable Correlation coefficient Correlation coefficient PREDICTORS: HSQ-Severity -0.606 -0.931 -0.751 -0.641 Number of Stressors 0.036 0.760 -0.524 -0.507 Parent Depression -0.507 —0.448 -0.110 0.312 SES 0.125 -0.208 -0.288 0.280 1.Va|, LG 0.458 0.356 -0.634 -0.369 2. SE, OES, Sch 0.494 0.228 -0.166 -0.092 3. PGE, PGD -0.211 0.438 0.109 0.089 4. PGE 0.000 -0.224 0.187 0.129 5. Implicit 0.037 0.644 -0.235 -0.361 OUTCOMES: Post- CBC—Extemalizing -0.394 0.271 -0.822 -0.019 CBC-Internalizing -0.807 -1.127 -0.494 -0.277 Long-Term- CBC-Externalizing -0.185 0.295 -0.884 -1.358 CBC-lntemalizing -0.500 0.077 -0.446 0.794 Parent Satisfaction TAI-1 0.110 -0.209 0.146 -0.005 TAI-2 0.591 0.531 -0.013 -0.144 [ = .950, p< .016 171 Table 34 Canonical Analysis of Predictors a_nd Outcome Variables in PMT Standardized Canonical Coefficient Variable Traditional Model Motivational Model Combined Model Root 1 Root 2 Root 1 Root 2 Root 1 Root 2 PREDICTORS: Traditional Predictors HSQ-Severity 0.995 -0.098 ------------ -0.931 -0.641 Number of Stressors -0.254 -1.034 ------------ 0.760 -0.507 Parent Depression 0.154 0.567 ------------ -0.448 0.312 SES 0.066 0.151 ------------ -0.208 0.280 Motivational Factors 1.Val, LG ------------ 0.752 -0.255 0.356 -0.369 2. SE, OES, Sch ------------ 0.354 -0.247 0.228 -0.092 3. PGE, PGD ------------ -0.047 -0.292 0.438 0.089 4. PGE ------------ -0.238 -0.084 -0.224 0.129 5. Implicit ------------ 0.306 0.811 0.644 -0.361 OUTCOMES: Child Behaviour Changes Post- CBC—Extemalizing 0.044 0.929 0.411 1.461 0.271 -0.019 CBC-lntemalizing 0.984 0.021 -0.477 -1.077 -1.127 -0.277 Long-Term- CBC-Externalizing 0.620 -1.914 1.020 -0.825 0.295 -1.358 CBC-lntemalizing -0.662 0.827 -0.675 0.630 0.077 0.794 Parent Satisfaction TAI-1 0.457 0.609 0.246 1.057 -0.209 -0.005 TAI-2 -0.380 -0.219 0.418 -0.873 0.531 —0.144 1 Model One: = .816, p < .018 2 Model Two I = .795, p < .280 3 Combined Model [ = .950, p < .016 172 one of the canonical variates accounted for a significant relationship between the two sets of variables (L: .942, p < .008), the second, not significant variate ([ = .799, p < .382), is also presented in Table 35. Highest loadings for the predictor set on the first variate include severity of child behavior, number of stressors, factor 5 and factor 3. All of the other variables (parent depression, factors 1,2, and 4) also contribute to the equation with modest loadings. Highest loadings for the outcome set on the first variate include changes in child behavior on internalizing behavior problems, externalizing problems, and parent satisfaction with the program (Table 35). Highest loadings for the predictor set on the second variate include severity of child behavior, factor 5, number of stressors, factor 1, parent depression. Highest loadings for the outcome set on the second variate include long term externalizing and internalizing problems (Table 35). Canonical variates from one set of variables are subject to change with any variations in the second set (Levine, 1984). Thus, it was decided to vary the nature of the second set. The variables in set 2 were changed by using child behavior change scores in lieu of the post and long term t-scores across scales (Tables 36 and 37). Neither of the first variates for the traditional model (r = .684, p < .168) or motivational model (r = .782, p < .081) was significant (Table 36). However, the first variate for the combined model was significant (r = .908, p < .028) (Table 38). The standardized coefficients indicate links worth identifying among the predictors and outcomes. Three of the traditional predictors (stress, severity of child behavior, and SES) and two of the motivational factors ( 1 and 5) are highly correlated with the first variate while one traditional (parent 173 Table 35 Correlgtions a_md Standardized Ca7nonical Coefficients Between Traditionai yd Motivational7Predictors and PMT Outcome Variables a_nd Their Cfianonical Variates in a Combined Model without SES First Variate Second Variate Canonical Canonical Variable Correlation coefficient Correlation coefficient PREDICTORS: Traditional Predictors HSQ-Severity -0.664 -0.985 0.695 0.561 Number of Stressors -0.020 0.610 0.477 -0.243 Parent Depression -0.523 -0.361 0.048 0.370 SES ---- ---- ---- ---- Motivational Factors 1.Val, LG 0.412 0.295 0.675 0.352 2. SE, OES, Sch 0.494 0.249 0.236 0.161 3. PGE, PGD -0.223 0.453 -0.180 -0.056 4. PGE 0.012 -0.245 -0.194 -0.199 5. Implicit 0.050 0.583 0.321 0.379 OUTCOMES: Child Behaviour Post- CBC-Externalizing -0.464 0.315 0.777 0.160 CBC-lntemalizing -0.854 -1.184 0.408 0.093 Long-Tenn- CBC-Extemalizing -0.256 0.178 0.863 1.343 CBC-lntemalizing -0.535 0.133 0.403 07% Parent Satisfaction TAI-1 0.164 -0.134 -0.035 0.164 TAI-2 0.585 0.468 0.056 0.078 r: .942 , p < .008 174 Table 36 Correlations. Standardized Canonical Coefficients a_nd Their Canonical Variates in Traditional and Motivational Models First Variate Second Variate Canonical Canonical Variable Correlation coefficient Correlation coefficient laditioneLModel One PREDICTORS: HSQ-Severity -0.059 -0.410 0.464 0.652 Number of Stressors 0.871 1.172 0.313 0.077 Parent Depression 0.107 -0.077 -0.530 -0.728 SES 0.118 -0.315 0.688 0.418 OUTCOMES: Child Behaviour Changes Pre- Post Change Score CBC-Extemalizing —0.580 -0.498 0.152 0.920 CBC-lntemalizing -0.103 0.584 -0.184 0417 Pre- LongTerm Change Score CBC-Externalizing -0.781 -0.589 -0.389 -0.550 CBC-lntemalizing -0.330 -0.122 -0.539 -0.880 Parent Satisfaction TAI-1 -0.437 -0.624 0.195 0.528 TAI-2 -0.006 0.267 0.035 -0.213 Motivational Model Two PREDICTORS: 1.Val, LG 0724 -0.629 0.515 0.483 2. SE, OES, Sch -0.370 0231 0.378 0.176 3. PGE, PGD 0.123 0.175 -0.357 -0.024 4. PGE 0.399 0.403 0.057 0.011 5. Implicit 0.506 0.547 0.825 0.818 OUTCOMES: Child Behaviour Changes Pre- Post Change Score CBC-Extemalizing 0.561 0.663 0.449 0.484 CBC-lntemalizing 0.016 -0.615 0.349 0.923 Pre- LongTerrn Change Score CBC-Extemalizing 0.669 0.377 0.093 0.135 CBC-lntemalizing 0.207 0.153 -0.207 -1.417 Parent Satisfaction TAI-1 -0.073 0.254 0.327 0.398 TAI-2 -0.590 -0.633 0.267 0.093 Model One 5 = .684, p < .168 Model Two 5 = .782, p < .081 175 Table 37 Correlations. Standardized Canonical Coefficients and Their Camnical Variates in a Combined Model First Variate Second Variate Canonical Canonical Variable Correlation coefficient Correlation coefficient PREDICTORS: HSQ-Severity -0.237 -0.559 0.294 0.197 Number of Stressors 0.338 0.686 0.002 0.680 Parent Depression -0.244 0.301 -0.361 -0.742 SES 0.076 -0.444 0.432 0.250 1.Val, LG 0.768 0.751 0.021 -0.505 2. SE, OES, Sch 0.395 0.099 0.239 0.292 3. PGE, PGD -0.158 0.180 -0.216 0.393 4. PGE -0.201 -0.271 -0.104 -0.343 5. Implicit -0.129 0.361 0.603 0.920 OUTCOMES: Child Behaviour Changes Pre— Post Change Score CBC-Externalizing -0.254 -0.307 0.253 1.181 CBC-Internalizing 0.143 0.977 -0.185 0537 Pre- LongTerm Change Score CBC-Externalizing -0.274 -O.210 -0.259 -0.559 CBC-lntemalizing -0.476 -0.675 -0.452 -0.867 Parent Satisfaction TAI-1 0.021 -0.415 0.090 0.532 TAI-2 0.636 0.795 -0.081 -0.217 g = .908, p < .028 176 Table 38 Canonical Anglvsis of Predictors fl! Outcome Variables in PMT Standardized Canonical Coefficient Variable Traditional Model Motivational Model Combined Model Root 1 Root 2 Root 1 Root 2 Root 1 Root 2 PREDICTORS: Traditional Predictors HSQ-Severity -0.410 0.652 ------------ -0.559 0.197 Number of Stressors 1.172 0.077 ------------ 0.686 0.680 Parent Depression -0.077 -0.728 ------------ 0.301 -0.742 SES -0.315 0.418 ------------ -0.444 0.250 Motivational Factors 1.Val, LG ------------ -0.629 0.483 0.751 -0.505 2. SE, OES, Sch ------------ -0.231 0.176 0.099 0.292 3. PGE, PGD ------------ 0.175 -0.024 0.180 0.393 4. PGE ------------ 0.403 0.011 -0.271 -0.343 5. Implicit ------------ 0.547 0.818 0.361 0.920 OUTCOMES: Child Behaviour Changes Pre- minus Post- CBC-Extemalizing -0.498 0.920 0.663 0.484 -0.307 1.181 CBC-lntemalizing 0.584 -0.417 -0.615 0.923 0.977 0537 Pre- minus Long-Term- CBC—Extemalizing -0.589 -0.550 0.377 0.135 -0.210 -0.559 CBC-Internalizing -0. 122 -0.880 0.153 -1.417 -0.675 -0.867 Parent Satisfaction TAI-1 -0.624 0.528 0.254 0.398 -0.415 0.532 TAI-2 0.267 -0.213 -0.633 0.093 0.795 0217 1 Model One; = .684, p< .168 2 Model Two 1 = .782, p < .081 3 Combined Model 1 = .908, p < .028 177 depression) and motivational predictor (factor 4) are moderately correlated. Within the second set, both internalized behavior change scores (post and long term-) as well as both parent satisfaction factors are highly correlated with the first variate. Thus, in comparing the three models only the combined model yielded a significant canonical variate (Table 38). The results of the second analysis support Hypothesis 2c that a combination of traditionally examined and motivational predictors would be most predictive of outcomes in PMT but do not provide support for Hypotheses 2a and 2b that models of traditional predictors and motivational predictors, respectively, would predict outcomes. Analysis Ill: Classifying Accordinq to Drop Out To test the hypothesis that PMT predictors would predict parent engagement, the third analysis (discriminant function analysis) described a linear combination of the predictor variables that best separated groups according to point of drop, i.e., at orientation (pre-PMT), mid-way through the PMT program (pari-PMT), or after long term follow up (completers). Table 39 contains group means and standard deviations for traditional predictors and motivational predictors. Note that the orientation drop out group had higher mean scores on number of stressors, parent depression, and Hollingshead SES than the other two groups. Also note that higher Hollingshead Index scores actually reflect lower SES. The proportion of single parent families was also higher for the orientation drop out group, next highest for the midway drop out group, and lowest for the completer group. The proportion of low income 178 Table 39 Means and Standard Deviations of Predictor Variables as a Function of PMT DropOut Point Point of Drop Outs Pre-PMT Pari-PMT Completers Predictor Variable M g M S___D M S79 Editiongl Predictors HSQ-Severity 4.49 1.58 4.56 2.24 4.55 2.21 Number of Stressors 4.44 2.10 3.32 2.29 2.77 2.28 Parent Depression 16.06 12.15 13.50 10.10 11.34 10.41 SES 51.69 15.23 45.00 13.37 43.49 14.94 Single Parent Family 0.50 0.52 0.38 0.52 0.29 0.46 Low Income Cutoff 0.38 0.50 0.88 0.35 0.74 0.44 Motivational Predictors 1.Val, LG 0.18 1.09 0.52 0.86 0.40 0.70 2. SE. OES, Sch -0.28 1.05 -0.74 0.98 -0.67 0.93 3. PGE, PGD -0.05 1.22 -0.45 1.01 -0.63 1.00 4. PGE 0.09 1.35 0.17 1.10 -0.22 0.89 5. Implicit -0.03 1.18 -0.07 0.67 0.22 1.31 Note 1. Single Parent Family is coded 0 for no (married, step-parent, common-law, etc.) and 1 for yes (single)and Low Income Cutoff is coded 0 for low and 1 for high. Thus, the mean of these variables is theproportion of cases with a value of 1. Note 2. Motivational predictors are standardized factor scores, z-scores with a mean of 0 and standard deviation of 1. 179 was highest for orientation drop outs and next highest for completers. The midpoint drop out group had the lowest proportion of low income among the three groups. Among the three groups, the completer group had the lowest performance goal and the highest implicit theorist mean scores; the pari- drop out group had the lowest self efficacy/optimistic explanatory style/schematicity, lowest implicit theorist, highest value and learning, and highest self enhancing performance goal mean scores; and, the orientation drop out group had the lowest value/learning goal, highest self efficacy/optimistic explanatory style/schematicity, and highest self defeating performance goal mean scores. Traditional Model The measure of the degree of association between the traditional discriminant scores and the drop out groups was not significant for either the first function (p < .125) or the second function (p < .676). Correlations between the values of the functions and values of the variables indicated that low income cutoff had the highest correlation with the discriminant function (.769), number of stressors had the second largest correlation (.544), and single parent family had the third largest (-.360) in absolute value (Table 40). The latter’s negative value indicates that small function values are associated with the presence of single parent families and high values are associated with non-single parent households. These results are similar to those obtained by the standardized coefficients (Tables 41 8- 42). The discriminant function for the traditional model correctly classified 64.4% of the originally grouped cases (Table 43). Of 35 parents who completed 180 Table 40 Wes in Discriminant Fu_nction Analysis by Traditional, Motivational, and Combined Models Equivalent Significance Predictor Variables Wilks's Lambda F(df ) IgditionaJ Predictor; HSQ-Severity 1.000 0.005 0.995 Number of Stressors 0.898 3.166 0.050 Parent Depression 0.964 1.061 0.353 Low Income Cutoff 0.856 4.727 0.013 Single Parent Family 0.962 1.092 0.343 Motivatlmal Predickg 1.Val, LG 0.981 0.489 0.616 2. SE, OES, Sch 0.966 0.900 0.413 3. PGE, PGD 0.980 0.525 0.595 4. PGE 0.975 0.655 0.524 5. Implicit 0.988 0.304 0.739 Combined Model HSQ-Severity 0.989 0.262 0.771 Number of Stressors 0.924 1.972 0.150 Parent Depression 0.937 1 .162 0.209 Low Income Cutoff 0.914 2.249 0.116 Single Parent Family 0.963 0.913 0.408 1.Val, LG 0.972 0.700 0.502 2. SE, OES, Sch 0.973 0.666 0.518 3. PGE, PGD 0.979 0.520 0.598 4. PGE 0.957 1.083 0.347 5. Implicit 0.993 0.158 0.854 Note. Degrees of freedom for Traditional Model, Motivational Model, and Combined Model are F(2, 56), F (2, 51), and F (2, 48), respectively. *p<.05 181 Table 41 Correlation of Predictor Variables with Discnmmrit Functions (Function Structgre Matrix) and Standardized Discriminant Fugion Coefficients Correlation with Standardized discriminant discriminant function function coefficients Predictor Variable Function 1 Function 2 Function 1 Function 2 mitim Predictors HSQ-Severity 0.026 0.008 0.518 -0.063 Number of Stressors -0.610 0.544 -0.451 0.617 Parent Depression -0.345 0.364 -0.393 0.296 Low Income Cutoff 0.769 0.470 0.778 0.860 Single Parent Family 0360 0.305 0.062 0.503 Motivational Predictors 1.Val, LG 0436 0.234 -0.493 0.271 2. SE, OES, Sch 0.612 -0.107 0.690 -0.275 3. PGE, PGD 0.464 -0.135 0.514 -0.254 4. PGE 0.344 0.759 0.118 0.857 5. Implicit -0.245 -0.499 -0.340 0446 Combined Predictors HSQ-Severity -0.055 -0.300 -0.345 -0.320 Number of Stressors 0.486 0.250 0.531 0.570 Parent Depression 0.451 -0.148 0.499 0172 Low Income Cutoff -0.481 0.423 -0.316 0.553 Single Parent Family 0.345 -0.028 0.145 0.133 1.Val, LG 0277 0.205 -0.543 0.230 2. SE, OES, Sch 0.264 -0.223 0.204 -0.352 3. PGE, PGD 0.245 -0.151 0.119 0.005 4. PGE 0.217 0.522 0.267 0.795 5. Implicit -0.082 -0.201 -0.050 0.171 182 Table 42 Correlations Between Discriminating Variables and Discriminant Functions (Function Structure Matrix) Variable Function 1 Function 2 T_raditional Predictors HSQ-Severity 0.026 * 0.008 Number of Stressors -0.610 * 0.544 Parent Depression -0.345 0.364 * Low Income Cutoff 0.769 * 0.470 Single Parent Family 0360 * 0.305 Motivational Factors 1.VaI, LG 0436 * 0.234 2. SE, OES, Sch 0.612 * -0.107 3. PGE, PGD 0.464 * -0.135 4. PGE 0.344 0.759 * 5. Implicit -0.245 -0.499 * Combined Model HSQ-Severity -0.055 -0.300 * Number of Stressors 0.486 * 0.250 Parent Depression 0.451 * -0.148 SES -0.481 * 0.423 Single Parent Family 0.345 * -0.028 1.Val, LG 0277 * 0.205 2. SE, OES, Sch 0.264 * -0.223 3. PGE, PGD 0.245 * -0.151 4. PGE 0.217 0.522 * 5. Implicit -0.082 -0.201 * 183 * largest absolute correlation between each variable and any discriminant function . axe «..00 u 00000 000.000... 2.00000 .0 000.000.00 ..0.0>O .0.0z 0.00 .0 o o v. . . v 00 .0.000..00 0.00 .. o 0 0...... . 0 so 020 020-000 0.00 0 o o 0.0.. N 0. .8 00.0 02.0.0000 oxo 0- .x. 0. 0.. m 0 0.00.000.0E 000.0 .0:.0< .0.0_0E00 ....20-..00 00..0.00..O 0.00.00E0s. 000.0 00.0.8.0 >.0O 0.0.0.00.fl.000...00.... 00.0: .00 00.0 .0. 0.0>.-00< 0000050006 0? 0.00..- 184 the program, 31 were predicted correctly to be members of the completer group (88.6 %), while 4 were assigned incorrectly to the orientation drop out group (11.4 %). Seven (43.8 %) of the parents who dropped out at orientation were classified correctly while 4 (11.4 %) were misclassified as completing the program and 1 (12.5 %) was misclassified as dropping out midway through the program. Even worse, 0 (0%) of parents who dropped out mid-way through PMT were correctly classified while 1 (12.5 %) was misclassified as dropping out at orientation, and 7 (87.5 %) were misclassified as completing the program. From Figure 9, we can see the boundaries which mark off the combination of function values that result in the classification of the cases into two of the three groups, i.e., there are no boundaries for the pari-PMT drop out group. To put these results into decision-making language, the use of the TEP model to predict PMT drop out outcomes would result in 6.8% false positives, 27.1 % false negatives, and 66.1 % true/correct. Severity of child behavior and parent depression had the highest correlation coefficient ( r = .424); and, low income cuttoff and single parent family have the next highest correlation ( r = -.416). lnterdependencies among the variables appear to be minimal; thus, an interdependency effect on this multivariate analysis was minimal. Motivational MM The measure of the degree of association between the motivational discriminant scores and the drop out groups was also not significant for either the first function (p < .292) or the second function (p < .157). Correlations between 185 Canonical Discriminant Function 2 -3.0 —2.0 -1.0 .o 1.0 2.0 .o 1. 1 1 1 1 1 1 T l i l T 3.0 i 12 12 12 12 12 12 -~ + + +12 + + 12 12 12 12 12 -- + + +12 + + 12 12 12 12 12 .o 4 + + * 1132 * + + 1133 32 1133* 32 1133 32 133 32 113 32 -1.0 4 + 1133 + 32+ + 1133 32 1133 32 1133 32 1133 32 133 32 -2.0 - 113 + + + 32 + 1133 32 1133 32 1133 32 1133 32 33 32 -3.0 1 32 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 T i l l l 1 i -3.0 -2.0 -1.0 .o 1.0 2.0 .o Canonical Discriminant Function 1 Figure 9 — Territorial Map with Traditional Model 186 the values of the functions and values of the variables indicated that factor 2 had the highest correlation with the discriminant function (.612), factor 3 had the second largest correlation (.464), and factor 1 the third largest (-.436) in absolute value (Table 41). The latter’s negative value indicates that small function values are associated with the presence of higher value and learning goals and larger values are associated with the absence of value and learning goals. These results are also similar to those obtained by the standardized coefficients (Table 42). The discriminant function for the motivational model correctly classified 64.8% of the originally grouped cases (Table 44). Of 33 parents who completed the program, 31 were predicted correctly to be members of the completer group (93.9%), while 2 were assigned incorrectly to the orientation drop out group (6.1 %). Four (26.7%) of the parents who dropped out at orientation were classified correctly while 11 (73.3 %) were misclassified. Again, 0 (0%) of the parents who dropped out mid-way through PMT were correctly classified while 1 (16.7 %) was misclassified as dropping out at orientation, and 5 (83.3%) were misclassified as completing the program. The numbered boundaries mark off the combination of function values that result in the classification of parents into the three groups (Figure 10). To put these results into decision-making language, the use of the motivational model to predict PMT drop out outcomes would result in 3.7% false positives, 29.6 % false negatives, and 66.7 % true. 187 .0.. 0.3 u 00000 0050000 2.00000 .0 000.0800 __0.0>0 .0.02 0.00 .0 o o ..0 0 00 0.00050 0.00 0 o o 0.0. . 0 5o 090 020-000 0.2 I o 0 Now 0 m. .00 005 02.0.0000 .0.. m .0.. m .00 m 0 200000.000. 0:05 _0:.0< 00.0.0800 0.5.0-000 02.0.0000 2000000002 000.0 00.20000 >_00 00.20000 .002.0>_.0_2 020: .30 005 00. 0_0>_00< 0000050020 3 030... 188 Canonical Discriminant Function 2 -3.0 -2.0 -1.0 .0 1.0 2.0 .0 l l l l l l l I I I I I 4T 3.0 w 3 32 3 13333 111133333 1.0 0 111113333 + + 111133333 111113333 111133333 111113333 1111 -o + + + + 1» -1.0 0 + + + l I 1* 1 1 . 0 -2.0 -1.0 .0 332 3322 12 12 12 12 12 12 12 12 12 12 12 12 12 12 12 12 12 12 12 l I 1.0 Canonical Discriminant Function 1 2. Figure 10 — Territorial Map with Motivational Model 189 + o-+— .CD—II— Since factor 2 and 4 have the largest correlation coefficient (.169) interdependencies among the variables appear to be minimal; thus, an interdependency affect on this multivariate analysis was minimal. However in the motivational model, Box’s M F test indicated significant results (p < .003), causing us to reject the null hypothesis of equal population covariance matrices and conclude that one of the assumptions of discriminant analysis was violated. Combined Model The measure of the degree of association between the discriminant scores and the drop out groups was also not significant for the combined model. The significance level for the first function was .678, indicating that it did not contribute substantially to group differences. Correlations between the values of the functions and values of the variables indicated that number of stressors had the highest correlation with the discriminant function (-.543), low income cutoff had the second largest correlation (-.481), and parent depression had the third largest (.451) (Table 42). These results are also similar to those obtained by the standardized coefficients (Table 41). The discriminant function for the combined model correctly classified 62.7 % of the originally grouped cases (Table 45). 0f 31 parents who completed the program, 27 were predicted correctly to be members of the completer group (77.4 %), while 3 were assigned incorrectly to the orientation drop out group (6.1 %) and 1 to the pan-PMT group (6.5 %). Five (28.6 %) of the parents who dropped out at orientation were classified correctly while 9 (64.3 %) were misclassified. Once more, 0 (0%) of parents who dropped out mid-way through 190 . .0.. fine u 00000 0000020 2.00000 00 00000800 __000>0 .0.02 E0 00 0.0 . 0.0 0 .0 00.200000 0.00 0 o o 0.0. . 0 so 02.0 020-000 0.00 0 o o 0.00 0 3 .8 02.0 02.0.0000 .00 m 00 m 00 m .10. 2000000000. 00000 _0:.0< 00.200000 020-000 02.0.0000 200009005. 0020 00.2005 0022 0020.000 0I0_ 000.2005 _002.0>_.0_2 000 00220000 0.0m 0203 .00 005 .0.. 20205.. 0000050005 mv 0B0... 191 PMT were correctly classified while 1 ( 28.6 %) was misclassified as dropping out at orientation, and 5 ( 83.3 %) were misclassified as completing the program. From Figure 11, we can see the numbered boundaries which mark off the combination of function values that result in the classification of parents into the three groups. To put these results into decision-making language, the use of the combined model to predict PMT drop out outcomes would result in 7.8 % false positives, 27.5 % false negatives, and 64.7 % true. Factor 3 and parent depression had the largest correlation coefficient, .325, factor 5 and single parent families had the next largest, .304, and then factor 3 and severity of child behavior, .276. Since these correlations were not particularly large, interdependencies among the variables appeared to be minimal once more; thus, an interdependency affect on this multivariate analysis was also minimal. However, Box’s Test of Equality of Covariance Matrices indicated significant results (p < .001), causing us to reject the null hypothesis of equal population covariance matrices and conclude that one of the assumptions of discriminant analysis was violated. The results of the third analysis do not support the hypothesis that the traditionally examined predictors, the motivational predictors, and. a combination of both would predict parent engagement in PMT. Analysis IV: Individual Motivational Resources The fourth analysis (logistic regression) tested the hypothesis that the motivational aspect subscales would be useful in predicting parent drop out or 192 Canonical Discriminant Function 2 -3.0 —2.0 -1.0 o 1.0 2. .o l l l l l I I I I I I 3.0-1- 32 «1- 32 32 32 32 32 + + + +32 «- 32 32 32 32 32 + + + 32 + -- 32 32 * 32 32 33333333333333332 .0 «3333333333333333311111111111*1112 ‘ + v 11111111111111111 12 12 12 12 12 + + + 12 + ‘- 12 12 12 12 12 -2 o - + + + 12 + 0 12 12 12 12 12 -3.o~l 12 «l l. l J l l .1 I I I I I I -3.0 -2.0 -1.0 .0 1.0 2.0 .0 Canonical Discriminant Function 1 Figure 11 — Territorial Map with Combined Model 193 completion of the PMT program (See Appendix D for Rationale for Using Subscales). Table 46 provides regression coefficients and significance levels for the variables in the model. Variables with positive root values indicate that as the variable values increase, the likelihood of parents completing the program increases. For example, as self efficacy increases (p < .0491 ), the likelihood of parents completing the program increases and as pessimistic explanatory style (p < .0010) increases, the likelihood of parents completing the program decreases. Variables with small root values indicate the variable has a small partial contribution. For example, the low root values for optimistic explanatory style, learning goals, both performance goals, and schematicity indicate that these motivational aspects have a small partial contribution. Interaction effects indicate that the interaction between pessimistic explanatory style and value is significant ( p < .0007). Comparing predictions to the observed outcomes, 33 parents who completed the program were correctly predicted by the model not to dropout. Similarly, 19 parents who dropped out were correctly predicted to prematurely terminate. A total of 11 parents were incorrectly classified, 4 as drop outs and 7 as completers. Of the parents who did not drop out, i.e., completed the program, 89.19% were correctly classified. Of the parents who dropped out, 73.08 % were correctly classified. Overall, 82.54% of parents were correctly classified by the model. The results of the fourth analysis support the hypothesis that a combination of select individual motivational resources predict parent 194 Table 46 Logistic Regression Predicting PMT Drop Out (n=64) Varable Beta SE Odds Wald Sig. Ratio Statistic Self Efficacy 0.341 0.173 0.148 3.872 0.049 * Optimistic Explanatory Style 1.385 1.144 0.000 1.465 0.226 (OES) Pessimistic Explanatory Style -10.784 3.277 -0.322 10.832 0.001 *** (PES) Learning Goal -0.144 0.142 0.000 1.037 0.309 Performance Goal 0.129 0.134 0.000 0.928 0.335 -- self defeating Performance Goal 0.179 0.130 0.000 1.901 0.168 -- self enhancing Implicit Theory -0.324 0.173 -0.133 3.518 0.061 Schematicity -O.139 0.260 0.000 0.287 0.592 Value -O.633 0.322 -0.148 3.865 0.049 * OES * Value -0.035 0.025 0.000 1.891 0.169 PES * Value 0.268 0.079 0.332 11.891 0.001 *** * p_< .05 ** g < .01 *** p_ < .001 195 engagement, i.e., drop out. Furthermore, the results also support the hypothesis that an interaction between pessimistic explanatory style and value predicts parent drop out from PMT and thus, provides support for the expectanch value model of motivation. Analysis V: Univariate Examination of Discrete Variables The fifth analysis tested the hypothesis that two discrete motivational aspects (implicit theorist orientation and schematicity) as well as select demographic variables would be associated with drop out. This chi-square analysis identified isolated associations among predictors and the drop out outcomes. Approaching significant associations were found between point of drop out in parent training and 1) low income cutoffs (Chi-square = 8.188, df = 2, p < .017) and 2) early developmental milestones (Chi-square = 10.077, df = 4, p < .039) (Tables 47 and 48). Using a Bonferroni adjustment (p. < .004), however, indicates that these associations are not significant. All other chi-square analyses also revealed insignificant results (Table 49). The results of the fifth analysis do not support the hypothesis that parent implicit theorist orientation predicts parent drop out. The results also do not support the hypothesis that parents schematicity predicts drop out. However, the results do support the hypothesis that select demographic variables may be associated with drop out. 196 Table 47 Prevalence (%) of Statistics Canada Income Cutoffs within Points of Drop Out Point of Drop Out Cutoff No Pre- Pari- Chi- p (_ = 37) (g = 18) (_ = 9) Square Low 29.7 66.7 22.2 8.188 0.017 High 70.3 33.3 77.8 * p < .004 (with Bonferroni adjustment) Table 48 Prevalence (%) of Ea_rlv Developmental Milestones within Points of Drop Out Point of Drop Out Cutoff No Pre- Pari- Chi- Q U.) = 37) (fl = 18) (11 = 9) Square within normal limits 89.2 77.8 50.0 10.077 0.039 late 8.1 11.1 50.0 early 2.7 11.1 0.0 * p < .004 (with Bonferroni adjustment) 197 Table 49 Chi-Square Analyses of finicipgnt Char_a_cteristics within Points of Drop Out Characteristics Chi- df Q Square Parent Education 7.088 8 0.527 Parent Ethnicity 4.348 2 0.114 Current Annual Income 29.976 20 0.070 Social Class (Hollingshead) 7.641 8 0.469 Child Medication Usage 7.901 4 0.095 Single Parent Household 1.697 2 0.428 Child Diagnosis 14.768 10 0.141 Previous Treatments 3.245 8 0.918 Schematic/Aschematic 2.839 2 0.242 incremental/Implicit Theorist 0.307 2 0.858 * p < .004 (with Bonferroni adjustment) 198 Analysis VI: Changes in Parent Motivational Schema To test the hypothesis that parents in the treatment group would show change in motivational resources while parents in the control group would not show change as a result of participating in PMT, a series of t-tests were conducted (Tables 50 & 51). Parents in the treatment group demonstrated significant change from pre- to post- PMSS administration on the self efficacy and approaching significance on the schematicity subscales (Table 50). Parents in the control group demonstrated no significant differences on the pre- and post- subscales. Parents in the treatment group demonstrated significant change from pre— to post- PMSS administration on the factor scales (Table 51). The results support the hypothesis that treatment group participants would show change on motivational resources but only on select motivational resources (self efficacy and possibly, schematicity). The results support the hypothesis that the control group would show no change. 199 Table 50 Mean Motivational Scores and T-test for Pre- and Post- Subscales by Treatment (n=37) and Control Group (n=29) Subscale Score PMSS Pre- Post- Pair-wise Subscale Mean (Sd) Mean (Sd) T-Test Signif. Treatment SE 19.340 (2.45) 20.530 (2.53) -3.282 0.003 * OES 11.560 (2.37) 11.910 (2.90) -0.809 0.425 PES 3.310 (1.35) 3.340 (1.00) -0.120 0.905 LG 22.270 (3.29) 22.310 (3.62) -0.068 0.947 PGD 10.890 (3.44) 11.030 (6.38) -0.132 0.896 PGE 9.380 (2.95) 8.970 (3.11) 1.132 0.266 IMP 5.250 (2.17) 4.840 (1.92) 0.871 0.390 SCH 5.220 (1.52) 5.970 (1.45) -3.000 0.005 * VAL 42.280 (5.15) 41.310 (6.55) 0.935 0.357 Control SE 18.850 (2.34) 19.150 (2.82) -0.433 0.673 OES 11.310 (2.36) 11.460 (2.22) -0.237 0.817 PES 2.690 (0.85) 2.460 (0.66) 1.389 0.190 LG 19.230 (3.94) 19.150 (2.54) 0.078 0.939 PGD 11.230 (3.54) 10.150 (5.24) 0.735 0.476 PGE 8.380 (1.89) 8.310 (2.93) 0.114 p 0.911 IMP 5.620 (2.10) 5.770 (2.59) -0.213 0.835 SCH 6.380 (1.71) 5.310 (1.93) 2.344 0.037 VAL 33.150 (9.51) 32.540 (10.78) 0.273 0.789 *significance criteria of .0056 (with Bonferroni adjustment) 200 Table 51 Mean Motivational Scores and T-test for Pre- and Post- Factors by Treatment (n=37) and Control Group (n=29) Subscale Score PMSS Pre- Post- Pair-wise Factor Mean (Sd) Mean (Sd) T-Test Signif. Treatment LV 58.28 (4.71) 58.03 (4.47) -4.669 0.869 IMC 23.56 (7.57) 26.78 (8.66) 0.166 0.000 * PGD 11.31 (3.34) 11.61 (5.98) —0.356 0.724 PGE 5.51 (2.33) 5.05 (2.36) 1.536 0.133 IT 5.41 (2.31) 4.81 (1.88) 1.410 0.167 Control LV 47.08 (12.22) 46.00 (12.40) 0.399 0.697 IMC 25.07 (4.65) 24.71 (5.85) 0.324 0.751 PGD 11.23 (3.90) 11.46 (6.06) -0.170 0.868 PGE 5.43 (2.24) 4.93 (2.76) 1.202 0.251 IT 5.43 (2.14) 5.57 (2.59) -0.213 0.834 significance criteria of .01 (with Bonferroni adjustment) 201 Chapter 5 DISCUSSION Summary of Results The study findings support several of the overall hypothesis and do not support several others. Table 52 provides a summary of these findings. A post- hoc analysis reveals that power is low for the current study. As a result, low power may be a partial explanation for the nonsignificant results (see Appendix F for Power Analysis Summary). Hypothesis I: Treatment Effects The study findings do not formally support the hypothesis that treatment group participants will report improvements in child behavior in comparison to control group participants in PMT. However, the results did identify a significant main effect for the CBCL behavior scale and for the interaction between scale and group. Thus, it was established that ratings for each of the CBCL scales depend on the group variable. Several explanations are consistent with the lack of significant results in treatment effects. Only about half of the parents who complete PMT programs, report a significant change in child behavior (Johnston, 1996). While parent management sessions typically produce changes in child behavior as rated by parents, changes do not bring children within normal limits and are considerably weaker than changes produced from medication (Hinshaw et al., 1998). PMT in combination with social skills training did not find any significant difference 202 >._ own. .5 8323 S btm>om 9885 Go .mnEsc 0:0... 20326 .0 .mnEac 3323 ho 3:93 8399.8 cacomamzmm 298 new notoaqsm .828ch .6323 35 8:55 £20665 Co «69¢ 58 Co cozchEoo .0 20:92.8 cozofimzmm E98 new 5328 2:0 62.53% Co: $2550 8605 mEmcom co=m>=22 .n 5339.8 cozoflmzmm E23 was 09.20 notoaaam $2550 .03ch Eco 869a amt .m 2228.5 303528 E 86:65 3 =3 3.3:? oEooSo msosczcoo ._ m>oc< 3:98 65:8 899 EoEfioF a_nmeaam 6c votoaanm 6c .9903 PPN “maootm EoEfiot. _ £9285 co_m_ooo m_m>_m:< 5:98me 3639590. £8509»: 236228536 9928i 5:5 mco_m_omo ocm .mow>_mc< $66365an $8.:on 056on Go meEnw Nm .2an 203 6m mm 038:3: E: _m> . mum mun. mm 0CD: 0:0: 0:05 3:033 25:8 notoaazm E: notoaanm :98an E: notoaaam E: notoaazm E: 663 8:523 99535 :o_mm9a9 27.52 330:2 EmcEEQmB :ozoce Em:_E_._om_c 88:2 EmEEEmE 89:89 .mcozgzoE :_ 3:20 a mocotoaxo P.2d :_ 99:2th 2.3 9:93 So :9: :Es 3383mm 9m 203953 303985 .mE9mn. So no.6 869: 89:89 .m:o=m>=oE 3336:. So :9: 869: 92069: E womb Son E :ozmcfiEoo .0 So :9: E93 “069: mEocom :o=m>=o_2 .n So :9: 869: mm» .m 99069: mzo::_Eoo E 69069: on =3) :30 :93 23:9 oEooSo 9985 3:00 Nm 9an >_ 204 between parents who received training and those who did not (Finch, 1998). It was also noted that social skills training alone did not produce any significant differences between treatment and control groups (Finch, 1998). These results are surprising given the well established efficacy of PMT programs (Kazdin, 1997; Kazdin & Weisz, 1998). PMT programs which run less than 8 weeks are generally less successful (Barkley, 1997). The current PMT may have been too short and, thus, provided limited availability and time for the parents to implement and practice the strategies and techniques discussed. In fact, the better change scores from pre- and long term CBCL scales for the treatment group, support this latter interpretation. It is also possible that parents who are high on self efficacy are likely to show better child behavior change outcomes regardless of group. Parents who are more disadvantaged on traditionally examined predictors are likely to show worse child behavior change outcomes regardless of group. The significant scale main effect and scale by group interaction are also consistent with several explanations. The increased internalizing problem scale results among control group participants is a summary of both pre- and post- measures. It is possible that the control group experienced increased anxiety regarding the treatment over the wait time and that this increase in worry, frustration, and/or anticipation is translated to the parents' perceptions of child behavior. Parental internal states may have influenced ratings of their children. Furthermore, the high incidence of comorbidity among anxiety and ADHD and other disruptive behavior disorders (Stahl & Clarizio, 1999) is consistent with 205 these findings. This result may suggest that parents and children with disruptive behavior disorders may be at risk for further developing internalizing problems if left untreated. Comorbidity involving both internalizing and externalizing problems may partially explain why good outcome measures were not achieved as well as have implications for future choice of treatment (Stahl & Clarizio, 1999). Finally, a selection bias may have been in effect in group assignment due to the small sample size and provides an alternate explanation for this difference. Parent participants in one particular group spent considerable energy and resources discussing outside factors, especially school climate, principal philosophies, and teacher practices inside the classroom and during recess. These parents firmly believed that these factors were strongly influencing their children’s behavior and impacting on the efficacy of implementing new parenting skills. While acknowledging the importance of context, these comments also revealed perceptions consistent with several motivational aspects including self efficacy, pessimistic explanatory style, and incremental theories. The high drop out rate in the current study, discussed below, may also provide a partial explanation for the differences between treatment and control groups on scale. It may be that certain participants who dropped out of the treatment group may have differed from those who stayed on some very important dimensions/characterstics. The control group’s increases in behavior problems from pre- to post- measures are diametrically contrary to the literature which consistently demonstrates drops in behavior problem rating scores as a direct result of 206 retesting. In school psychology intervention research involving rating forms, participants have been observed to indicate lower ratings on a second measure (Barkley, 1997). This increase is also contrary to regression towards to the mean as a result of retesting. The present results may have differed from previous research because of possible differences in wait periods for Canadian and United States clinic populations. Many of the participants from both the treatment and wait list control groups had been on an extensively long wait list due to the universal access to health care in Canada. The control group participants may have had their hopes raised due to finally being seen but then experienced increased anxieties once placed on another wait list, as noted above. Hypotheses II, III, and IV LA” Predictors and Outcomes) Three comparisons of models (traditional, motivational, and combined predictors) were conducted for several multivariate statistical procedures. The combined models in the canonical analysis predicted significance after controlling for the traditional and motivational models, respectively, suggesting that it may be possible to make a difference in PMT outcomes by considering and eventually manipulating the more dynamic motivational predictors. The results of the canonical correlation supports the hypothesis about the relationship between the two sets of variables for a sample of parents participating in a parent management training program. One set of variables measured traditionally examined predictors and motivational predictors and the 207 other set measured outcomes of PMT. The traditional predictors with the largest impact included severity of child behavior, parent depression, and number of psychosocial stressors. The motivational predictors included the five new motivational factors (LV, IMC, PGE, and IT). The outcome variables included child behavior change and parent satisfaction measures. While the results determined that motivational predictors alone were not significantly related to outcomes, weights assigned to coefficients suggest that certain predictors have more impact and loadings than other predictors on outcomes of PMT for a sample of parents at an outpatient health care organization. Brestan et al. (1999) found that parent expression of satisfaction may be more closely linked to symptom changes during treatment than to the levels of child behavior problems after treatment as measured by absolute scales. These findings are consistent with the stronger relations in the models utilizing behavior change scores. Furthermore, correlations between parent satisfaction and behavior problem severity were not significant in previous research studies (Brestan et al., 1999). The results of the discriminant analyses suggest that parents who are at high risk for dropping out of PMT cannot be identified early using the predictors in the current study. The discriminant function analysis failed to significantly classify group membership in the traditional predictor model, motivational predictor model, or combined model. However, classification percentages across traditional, motivational, and combined models suggested good rates for predicting completers. (88.6%, 93.9%, and 87.1 %, respectively), and adequate 208 rates for predicting orientation drop outs (56.3%, 73.3%, and 64.3%, respectively). It is predicting this latter group and identifying most cases of the smaller group which holds the most value. For example, by identifying everyone to be completers of a PMT program, the error rate will be very small, since fewer people actually drop out. However, the results are less valuable, since the goal is to identify the drop out parents. The intent of the disriminant analysis was to identify parents to receive special, extended treatment prior to PMT. Drop out rates are reportedly high in PMT, with estimates ranging from 25 per cent, 40-60 per cent, and 46.86 per cent, respectively (Johnston, 1996; Kazdin, et al., 1997; Wierzbicki & Pekarik, 1993). Some of the reasons for attrition and drop out have included subject geographical re-location and reduction in motivation (Peterson & Bell-Dolan, 1995) and identified social emotional disorders as at high risk (Fomess & Hoagwood, 1993). Attempts at preventing drop outs through management of the more static variables included minimal payments, accommodating parent shift-work, and establishing special pretreatment techniques to establish a relationship between experimenter and participants at orientation (Peterson & Bell-Dolan, 1995). Similar attempts to prevent drop outs in the current study were not particularly successful given the high drop out rate (42%). It is possible that the high drop out rates demonstrate the need for additional treatment. Attrition refers to important differences among participants who drop out of one group, participants who drop out of other groups, and/or participants who remain in the study (AATBS, 1995). This attrition is especially problematic in 209 longitudinal research studies in which the final sample is not comparable to the initial sample because of characteristics of the participants who dropped out (AATBS, 1995). Furthermore, attrition increases the probability of making a Type II error (in which no differences are found when differences exist) due to loss of power (Black & Holden, 1995). While it was hoped that the discriminant analysis would aid in the prediction of drop outs and it did not, the results do not preclude the use of motivational strategies and techniques to prevent premature termination. Mean scores suggest lower levels of value and Ieaming goals and higher levels of performance goals among drop out participants. Special treatment and care continues to need to be administered and can be tailored to target these motivational aspects with these parents to encourage completion of the program. The results of the logistic regression analysis support the hypothesis that original motivational aspects predict drop out among parents involved in PMT. Self efficacy, value, and pessimistic explanatory style appeared to have the most contribution to the model. The significant interaction between pessimistic explanatory style and value also supports the hypothesis of an expectancy times value effect. Hypothesis V (Individual Predictors) The results of the univariate analysis (chi-square) support the hypothesis that one traditional predictor (SES) was associated with drop out of PMT but the two other motivational predictors measured (schematicity and implicit theories) were not. The fact that SES failed to be clearly associated with outcomes in 210 many of the other multivariate analyses suggests that SES as a predictor is not as important within the context of other factors. It is also possible that the effects of SES were masked due to associations with other variables. Finally, previous studies have noted that SES is associated with type of PMT program preferred, i.e., low SES parents prefer more behaviorally orientated programs such as Barkley’s Defiant Children (Wood & Baker, 1999). Hypothesis VI (Changes in Motivational Aspects due to Participatiory Some of the motivational mediators may have been implicitly influenced by the nature of the parent training program, itself. Similar to behavioral parent training interventions (Tucker et al., 1998), it is possible that the PMT intervention in the current study increased parenting self-efficacy through the following sources of information: a) mastery attainment as parents practiced newly learned skills using standard homework assignments; b) vicarious learning experiences through observing other parents and listening to group discussions; c) verbal reinforcement from other parents and the therapist. These sources of information appear to be important, inherent components of the PMT intervention and may have lead to changes in parent motivational resources. For example, the PMT program was conducted with a group of similar parents. These parents may have provided coping models in the role plays and other activities, and thus functioned as effective strategies for increasing self efficacy beliefs. The informative nature of PMT also implicitly enhances self efficacy. This provides a strong argument for including strategies which explicitly address motivating mediators. These motivational enhancing strategies can be easily included in PMT programs. 211 The argument that the nature of PMT itself predisposes parents to a pessimistic explanatory style can be easily countered. Granted, once their child is diagnosed / labeled with a disorder, an entire gamut of motivational resources come into play. Many programs explicitly educate parents that ADHD is neurodevelopmental and therefore sets the parent up for an internal, stable, uncontrollable explanatory style which is pessimistic but possibly, realistic. However, the behavioral manifestations of ADHD relate to similar causal dimensions relevant to the other disruptive behavior disorders, as follows. Before beginning PMT, many parents are explicitly counselled that ODD and CD are largely environmental and can be influenced by factors such as parental consistency and predictability, and monitoring and tracking of behavior, respectively (Barkley, 1997). Ultimately, this practice sets the stage for a more external, unstable, and controllable, therefore, optimistic explanatory style as well as for a more incremental theorist orientation and increasing value for the therapy. It is this latter element, value for treatment which has been indirectly examined as a barrier to treatment (Kazdin & Wassell, 1999). Perceived relevance of treatment (which was defined as the extent to which treatment was seen as relevant to the child’s problems and met with parent expectations) was one of the sources of barriers which was strongly related to child behavior change (Kazdin & Wassell, 1999). Further Comments Overall, the traditional predictors appeared to have the largest and most significant impact on the outcomes measured in this study. Unfortunately, the 212 traditional predictors are primarily subject or demographic variables (Kazdin & Wassell, 1999). The traditional predictors can also be viewed as static predictors in that they represent aspects of the participants’ characteristics that are predictive of outcomes but are not believed to be as readily subject to change. Several of the motivational predictors in the current study also appeared to have an impact on outcomes but not to quite as clear an extent. The motivational predictors would have been more dynamic predictive factors which reflect the present circumstances and motivation of the participant. Thus, the motivational predictors may have the potential to be more readily amenable to change. Dynamic predictors tend to be more unreliable and may change over time. The lower reliability coefficients among the motivational aspects for the treatment group than for the control group suggests that the PMT may have been incidentally addressing some of the motivational aspects throughout the program. Because the control group did not have exposure to the PMT program at pre- and post- PMSS measurement points, their reliability estimates may be more reflective of the stability of the motivational scale. Measurements of the dynamic predictors involves a great deal of subjectivity. Any unreliability in measurement leads to underestimation of validity. Therefore, this line of reasoning implies that dynamic variables must be weaker predictors in comparison to static predictors. Because by their very nature motivational predictors are necessarily weaker and because manipulation of the traditional predictors did not enhance outcomes, arguments can still be made for the inclusion of motivational aspects in PMT. 213 The present study suggests that motivational aspects influence outcomes in PMT to the largest extent when traditionally examined predictors are considered. Recent evidence indicates that parent perception of few barriers to treatment acts as a protective factor for children identified as at risk for showing little behavior change because of low SES, parent psychopathology, stress, and severity of behavior (Kazdin & Wassell, 1999). Thus, the findings/ results do provide some minimal support for the main hypothesis that traditional and motivational aspects jointly predict outcomes in parent management training. Furthermore, these results provide support for recent motivational research arguments that both individual and environmental factors direct motivational energy (Maehr & Pintrich, 1999). The results highlight the potential impact that improvements in parent’s motivational aspects can have on children’s behavior changes, parent’s satisfaction with treatments, and parent engagement. The motivational aspect results provide tentative evidence for the inclusion of motivational modules in PMT and support for further theory-testing and refinement. Interest in the current study (with the exception of the chi-square analyses) focused on the joint performance of the set of variables at hand and reflects the mutual influence of the variables on each other. Considerable change in results was observed if some variables were added and others deleted. This change in results likely reflects reality and we want the variables to behave this way. To ask for the contribution of each variable independent of all 214 other variables is equivalent to requesting a univariate index that ignores the other variables (Rencher, 1995). Limitations of the Present Study Several methodological limitations in this intervention outcome study are acknowledged and at the same time corrective suggestions are presented. The purpose of the present section is to discuss methodology issues in the parent training intervention study. Some strategies and techniques for controlling these issues are included. The following limitations discussed include instrumentation, sampling issues, specificity, developmental considerations, treatment integrity, and reactivity. Instrumentation The present study focused on only a few Ieaming outcomes and some narrowly conceptualized measures. Consideration of a broader domain of outcomes than might be typically considered in PMT programs might reveal outcomes which can be far-reaching in nature and potentially effective. The present study attempted to measure some of these but simply did not measure or capture the entire gamut of outcomes. A recent case study of two children with ADHD/ODD included behavioral observations in the home in addition to the behavior rating scales of the current study; significant drops in child behavior problems were noted on CBCL and HSQ, as well as behavioral observations using the Behavior Management Flow chart in a PMT program (Danforth, 1999). Another recent case study directly observed outcomes of parent training in a multiple baseline design and evaluated change according to inappropriate child 215 behavior, compliance, on-task behavior, parent-child interaction, and a parent praise measure (Greene, Kamps, Wyble, & Ellis, 1999). Choice of specific instrumentation is related to this limitation in the present study. While one of the most popular outcome measures utilized in the treatment outcome research literature is Achenbach’s Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) and its related instruments (Drotar et al., 1995), the CBCL has many disadvantages as well as advantages. The advantages include sound psychometric properties regarding reliability and validity as an assessment of frequently presenting symptoms in children from 4-18 years of age; two large demographically diverse standardization samples (one referred and one non-referred); and ease of administration and scoring with a relatively inexpensive computer program (Drotar et al., 1995). The CBCL, being viewed as a broad-band scale, assesses many facets of behavior. One disadvantage is that it may be that only a couple of child behavioral areas might need change. The CBCL does not single out one or two target behaviors and could mask gains made in particular areas. As for other disadvantages, the CBCL has been noted to have not only limited sensitivity to milder, more subtle behavior problems but also to have compressed all raw scores within the normal range (less than 69th percentile) and assigned them the same T-score; provides a potentially misleading assessment of Social Competence which measures selected aspects of social functioning (social adjustment) and neglects others (social performance and social skills); is biased in reporting physical symptoms by not distinguishing organic from more 216 emotional and stress related disorders; provides no interpretative information regarding ethnically, culturally, and racially diverse populations; and, contains much subjectivity in scoring (Drotar et al., 1995). It is important to note that these issues are probably not unique to the CBCL and likely applicable to other measurement instruments and materials used in much intervention research. First and Way, (1995) discuss outcomes of PMT as facilitating the development of new ways of thinking, transformative learning, as well as enhancing specific parenting skills. The immediate goals of PMT are to develop very specific skills in parents (Kazdin, 1998) and thus, to basically “change” parenting behavior. No direct formal measure of parent behavior change was obtained in the current study. In fact, informal discussion with parents who were extremely satisfied with the program, discussed gains which were not explicitly measured in the program and which may be more related to reputation than behaviour change per se. Several parents commented on the need to separate the behavior from the child which intuitively implicates a movement from an implicit theory orientation to an incremental theorist orientation. Several parents commented on how much they found enjoying being with their children and that it was not a stressful time to dread and find ways to avoid. The home environment had becomemuch less stressful and amenable to positive interactions among family members. The present results suggest that given the potentially wide range of outcomes beyond mere changes in child behavior and satisfaction, and acquisition of parenting skills, therapists should consider motivational aspects more explicitly when 217 planning programs to create more meaningful experiences. In considering the issue of reputation versus behaviour change, it may be that reputation changes more slowly. Finally, the current study did not provide for any check of harmful outcomes. The PMT sessions had a heavy emphasis on positive reinforcement techniques in the first two weeks and the next two weeks included a punishment contingency (e.g., time out, response cost, fines). With families who are prone to negative, violent, and/or abusive interactions, it is possible that application of punishment contingencies may be difficulty (botched) and even harmful (Barkley, 2000) Thus, the present study may have measured a specific and narrow measure of outcome. Methodological rules of thumb are described as follows: to include multiple measures of treatment outcome in all relevant domains of any life situations likely to be influenced by treatment effectiveness (Peterson & Bell- Dolan, 1995); to assess areas of outcomes beyond the child domains (social functioning, academics) and include parent domains (interactions with child, stress, and marital discord) as well as broader contexts (family, living conditions, and neighborhoods) (Kazdin, 1995); to consider the use of multiple informants even though concordance among raters can be problematic (Tarullo et al., 1995). Forness and Hoagwood (1993) recommend a multiple diagnostic approach with several instruments. These recommended practices make instrumentation a particularly salient issue in school psychology intervention research; logistically, it would make sense that with more measures there is more opportunity for 218 instrumentation problems coupled with more opportunity to find significant effects. Sampling Issues The pre-test results suggest that the treatment and control groups may have differed prior to treatment on the CBCL scale ratings and several other characteristics. This lack of group equivalence (Peterson & Bell-Dolan, 1995) may be a reflection of selection bias or the small sample size. Selection refers to systematic differences between groups at the beginning of the research study which are the result of the method used to assign subjects to treatment groups or control groups (AATBS, 1995). In addition to systematic differences between groups another problem is having a non-representative sample of the population because some potential subjects failed to be included at all (Peterson 8. Bell-Dolan, 1995) and/or because some potential subjects did not sign the consent forms (Carlson, 1993; Eyberg, 1995). Based on estimates of referrals from the ADHD clinic, well over half of the parents who were referred for PMT did not initiate contact for an orientation session. Barkley (2000) reports that estimates of families failing to accept or attend parent training programs range from 30 % to 50 % or more. Thus, it continues to remain unknown how these families would fair with PMT. Based on records from priority/wait lists of referrals from physicians in the health care clinic, all of the parents who were referred attended orientation sessions; however, these orientation sessions doubled as initial interviews for upcoming evaluations and may have biased attendance. Three parents who 219 attended orientation sessions refused to participate in the research component of the program. Three other parents refused to participate in PMT at all. In any case, the participants in the study were representative of the population of interest, i.e., participants who typically participate in treatment (AATBS, 1995; Peterson 8. Bell-Dolan, 1995). As a result, the participants in the intervention study are basically a voluntary yet true clinical sample (Peterson & Bell-Dolan, 1995). For example, participants were essentially parents of children who show some clinical symptoms, children who meet clinical diagnosis, and children who are clinically referred, and non-referred clinical cases (Peterson & Bell-Dolan, 1995) Another systematic selection difference is the under-representation of children from diverse backgrounds and lack of inclusion of different ethnic groups in intervention research (Foster & Martinez, 1995). Fomess and Hoagwood (1993) included a comprehensive review which concluded that the literature is lacking in studying ethnic differences in intervention as well as assessment. Despite the fact that the US. population has been becoming increasingly pluralistic, theories of child behavior continue to be based on European American culture (Foster & Martinez, 1995). The population from which this sample was drawn is not particularly diverse but representative of small northern Canadian communities. These results do not generalize to larger, metropolitan, more diverse communities. Thus, the study has limited external validity which describes the generalizability of a research study’s results to other people, settings, and conditions (AATBS, 1995; Porter, 1988). 220 Random Assignment Random assignment was not total and thus, problematic in the clinic. A few parents who could not attend the upcoming group due to scheduling and other difficulties, were assigned to control groups. Sometimes, random assignment was not possible because of other considerations. An apt example of practical consideration is the situation in which staff from the Children’s Aid Society referred a parent with disruptive children for parenting sessions under court order and there were no other current parenting programs being offered in the community. In this case where random assignment was violated, the parent was excluded from the research data; however, it became apparent from informal conversations with CAS staff that other parents had been referred to their physicians and attended PMT under court order. However, some researchers have argued that a truly random sample where every individual in the population has an equal chance of being selected is never the case (Shulman, 1988). Henggeler, Smith, and Schoenwald (1994) provide two general persuasive arguments to overcome resistance to random assignment by decision makers who are concerned about unfairly depriving children and parents of services. First, empirical evidence indicates that treatments for serious antisocial youths has minimal effectiveness and reviews have concluded that nothing works (Henggeler et. al., 1994). Second, random assignment could be presented as the most ethical and fair method of allocating scarce treatment resources (Henggeler et. al., 1994). While these two arguments are presented as an 221 “ethical imperative", they can also be viewed as ethically questionable within the context of effective treatments and adequate resources. When participants have been involved in previous treatments (and sometimes concurrent treatments), the results of the study cannot be generalized to situations in which people will be exposed to the treatment under investigation. It should be noted that many of these factors (especially reactivity and multiple treatment interference) can be viewed as direct threats to both internal and external validity. Develpomental Considerations The present study chose not to consider the child’s age as a factor given limited research indicating age as a predictor is also strongly associated with severity of the disorder. It may have been less limiting to shrink the age range of the children in the study. Positive results of PMT may be manifested more quickly or easily when the children are younger (ages 5 to 7) than when they are older (10-12) (Barkley, 1997). If this is true, age of the child might have become conflated with the other variables of interest. However, given the small sample size, it was neither possible to restrict the age of children included in the study nor ensure that different sample subgroups contained similar age distributions. If true that severity of child behavior is strongly associated with age, then age as a factor is a moot point. However, type of child behavior disorder was also not examined in this cross-categorical approach study. Similar arguments/justifications to this limitation are applicable. Some research on CD indicates a hardening around age 8. 222 Subgroups and/or Comorbidity Considerations The present study failed to consider whether there were certain subgroups (especially subtypes of AD/HD) or comorbid conditions were related to parental drop out or other outcomes. Responsiveness to PMT may vary according to diagnosis, especially, subtype, and/or comorbidity (Barkley, 2000). A related limitation of that the present study seemed to neglect the child’s condition as a moderator variable (e.g., did parents of comorbid children differ from other parents?) Treatment Integrity The present study contained no formal measure providing assurance of treatment integrity which is the extent that treatment is actually implemented in the manner it was intended (Black & Holden, 1995; Shapiro, 1987). Specifically, the intervention did not contain objective observation of treatment delivery and documentation of the extent to which the intervention was implemented (Deschler & Schumaker, 1994). However, the study did contain set manualized procedures which were explicitly followed. These included a) a brief video tape presentation provided a summary and overview of procedures and strategies to be discussed prior to each session; b) objectively specified interventions and handouts; and, c) maintenance of a treatment log documenting subjective assessment of participant status, participants’ participation, procedures taught/discussed, and future plans for each session. 223 Reactivity Reactivity is a phenomenon in which research participants respond to an independent variable in a particular way simply because they know their behavior is being observed (AATBS, 1995, p.27). Reactivity can include four types of phenomena: 1) a Hawthorne Effect; 2) Evaluation Apprehension; 3) Demand Characteristics; and , 4) Experimenter Expectancy (AATBS, 1995). The Hawthome effect refers to the tendency of participants to perform better because of the attention they are receiving as research participants (AATBS, 1995). Evaluation apprehension causes participants to act in ways so as to avoid negative evaluations (AATBS, 1995). Demand characteristics include cues in the environment which inform participants of the purpose of the study or suggest to them what behaviors are expected of them (AATBS, 1995). Experimenter bias occurs when an experimenter unintentionally (usually) provides participants with cues (demand characteristics) that inform them of what behavior is expected of them (AATBS, 1995). Experimenters can act in ways that do not affect the participants directly but which can bias the results of the study (AATBS, 1995). The present study included no measure to objectively evaluate the effects of any reactivity. Pressley and Harris (1994) view sample size as the least controversial of methodological issues which should be planned in advance in the study design. Ultimately, it depends on the research question and a cost-benefit analysis of what kind of methodological strengths the experimenter prefers and what kinds of methodological weakness the experimenter will tolerate. The level of power in the 224 current study for detecting differences was low. Thus, it is possible that the study was not sensitive enough to detect differences which were being studied and/or the treatment was not strong enough for the statistics which are presented. Peterson and Bell-Dolan (1995) argue that it would make more sense to view methodological issues on a continuum rather as typically dichotomous, i.e., either the study has methodological flaws or does not. Specifically, we can conclude that the current study has elements of sound design features which are attributed to the inclusion of control groups, using multiple outcome measures, and collecting follow-up data (Dunak et al., 1995). The study used parents of children from existing clinical groups rather than from the general population. The study included theory based or developmentally rooted interventions. All of these features are in diametric opposition to limitations as posed in reviews of other intervention studies (Durlak et al., 1995). 225 Chapter 6 SUMMARY AND IMPLICATIONS The results speak to several of the research questions related to motivational resources. The motivational resources and value aspects formed identifiable motivational schema and several of these factors influenced outcomes but, for the most part, they did not. The motivational resources and value aspects grouped together to form five central schemas among individual parents (LV, IMC, PGE, and IT). Some of these schemas were predictive of some parent training outcomes (child behavior change and parent satisfaction with PMT). Furthermore, some of the individual motivational resources were predictive of parent engagement. Both the motivational schema and the individual motivational resources changed over time and were susceptible to greater change among treatment parents as a result of participation in a predominantly behaviorally oriented PMT. The latter suggests these resources are more readily amenable to therapeutic influence. Implications for Theory Motivation as a construct has largely been applied to educational theory. The current results may have implications for borrowing constructs from one setting (classroom) and transferring to another setting (clinical) and suggest these theoretical constructs are not readily transferable. 226 Implications for Practice The implication for future practice is that although therapists (like teachers) can do little aboUt an individual’s experience with uncontrollable outcomes and predictors (socioeconomic status, family constellation, psychosocial stressors), they can do much regarding how individuals come to interpret and understand why those outcomes occur (Reeve, 1996). There are numerous motivational aspects that have developed empirically validated methods to enhance academic achievement. The present study found that there are also several aspects of motivation (parent self-efficacy, schematicity, optimistic explanatory style, learning goals, performance goals, and value) which combined with traditional predictors can be successful in predicting various outcomes in parent training programs. These results suggest that proponents of parent management training programs consider diverting (or augmenting already considered) effort toward encouraging parents’ inner motivational resources in order to increase the probability of successful outcomes. Furthermore, programs with primary motivational goals and strategies which focus on the motivation to Ieam, the intention of acquiring the knowledge or skills that activities are intended to develop, are likely to be most successful (Brophy, 1998). The results of the present study suggest that useful heuristics/guidelines can be offered to therapists in providing a comprehensive PMT program targeted specifically at addressing, compensating for, and/or optimizing these significant motivational mediators in parent training outcomes. While some PMT programs 227 implicitly provide methods for enhancing motivational factors by their very nature, motivational mediated parent training would serve to make these more explicit. Schema Mediated Parent Training may be a useful adjunct to traditional PMT approaches. While the content of materials will be very similar, participants in each treatment group will be exposed to different methods of instruction based upon a set of heuristic guidelines. These heuristic guidelines can be viewed as “a set of rules of thumb that can be adapted to fit each situation, rather than a rigid procedure that must be followed in the same way every time” (Newby et al., 1996, p.120). Parent Efficacy and Outcomes in Practice For those parents who present with low self efficacy beliefs which significantly mediates negative outcomes in PMT, explicit guidance and instruction is warranted. Future practice may consider a comprehensive program for self efficacy training, consisting of three parts (identifying component skills, measuring self-efficacy expectations, and supplying positive self-efficacy information) and using the four sources of information (performance accomplishments, vicarious experience, verbal persuasion, and physiological states) (Reeve, 1996). Borrowing the guidelines from the seven-step procedures recommended for instructors and students, PMT may also incorporate them as follows: 1. Therapist identifies component skills needed for competent performance. Instructor measures parents’ self-efficacy expectations on each of those skills (possibly through using components of the PITS). 2. Therapist models each component skill. 3. Following the modeling, parents perform each component skill through role playing, and “home” work practice. 228 4. Individually mastered component skills are integrated into competent functioning during a simulation of the task (using various vignettes). Therapist graduates the simulations so they consist of a succession of increases in task difficulties and performance obstacles (vignettes organized in a hierarchy of problematic behavior). 5. While one parent performs the simulated task, other parents watch on the sidelines. Parents offer the performer skill-related tips and motivation-related encouragement. 6. Therapist provides additional instructive modeling, using corrective feedback as needed. 7. Therapist identifies stressful indicators and warning signs (increased heart rate, clenched jaw) and models confident demeanor and arousal regulating techniques (relaxation, imagery) to parents. (adapted from Reeve, 1996, p. 85) Teaching efficacy beliefs is both possible and important in that it can build strong and resilient cognitive beliefs that parents can use to counter the unpredictable, stressful, and difficult challenges of raising a child with disruptive behavior tendencies. Efficacy beliefs vary in how strong and resistant they are to change based on the amount of information which formed them (Reeve, 1996). For example, firm efficacy beliefs are based on much, consistent information and require compelling disconfirmation (Reeve, 1996). lnconclusive efficacy beliefs are based on little, inconsistent information and easily subject to reanalysis (Reeve, 1996). It is these latter beliefs which seem more characteristic of parents whose children have disruptive behavior disorders. Efficacy can be enhanced by the following therapist efforts in PMT: 1) Encouraging parents to set specific, difficult, and attainable goals; 2) Model and cue effective response strategies; 3) Provide feedback that helps students to achieve success; 4) Make attributional statements that help them to appreciate that they are developing their abilities by accepting challenges and applying consistent effort. (Brophy, 1998, p.57) 229 Parent Personal Control Beliefs and Practice Parents with pessimistic explanatory styles can also be explicitly taught to adapt an optimistic explanatory style in a therapy process consistent with “attributional retraining”. Attribution retraining was used to teach students to consider more adaptive attributional analysis to explain school’s aversive events (Reeve, 1996). The goal of attributional retraining programs is to learn to make intemal-stabIe-controllable attributions for successes and extemaI-unstable- uncontrollable attributions for failures (Reeve, 1996). This training is first accomplished by the therapist extending the range of possible attributions for failures and suggesting alternative attributions such as ineffective strategy, insufficient effort, lack of proper training or experience, high task difficulty (Reeve, 1996). Second, the therapist persuades parents to change their pessimistic attributions through three procedural techniques: 1) Persuasion: a parent performs a task and a therapist verbalizes the desired attribution and then attempts to convince the parent of the validity of the desired attribution; 2) Behavior modification: the therapist encourages parents to make attributions of ability, effort, task difficulty, and luck for failure but verbally reinforces only the unstable, uncontrollable attribution (e.g. luck); and, 3) Information: for example, therapists inform parents about the nature of their children’s disorders. (adapted from Reeve, 1996, p.111) This third avenue usually occurs outside of PMT. For example, many therapists may provide information regarding the nature of childhood disorders prior to beginning PMT and usually during assessment feedback sessions. Parents with a Ieamed helplessness orientation require support through two avenues: 1) changing a nonresponsive environment to a responsive one, 230 and 2) changing attributional style. The latter is easier and accomplished through attributional training and retraining programs discussed above. The former requires a systems approach and restructuring of the environment which is not always possible. Multisystemic treatment approaches may be more advantageous here (Henggeler, Schoenwald, Borduin, Rowland, & Cunningham, 1998). Furthermore, attributional retraining which explicitly teaches parents to attribute their successes to their ability as well as reasonable effort, and to attribute their failures to lack of infomtation or response strategies as well as lack of effort, (Brophy, 1998) is likely to promote more positive PMT outcomes. In previous attribution retraining treatments, like with students in achievement contexts, parents could be taught the following: 1. to concentrate on the task at hand and not worry about “failing” 2. to cope with “failures” by retracing their steps to find their mistake or by analyzing the problem and finding another approach 3. to attribute their failures to insufficient effort, lack of information or use of ineffective strategies (Brophy, 1998, p.86) Cunningham (1990) incorporates a number of strategies in his PMT community programs which foster social Ieaming attributions for strategy choices and child behavior. Among these strategies are the use of errorless Ieaming, problem solving, and coping models (Johnston, 1996). Attributional retraining has been incorporated as a component of many other therapies for example, Beck’s psychotherapy, Baucom and Epstein’s marital therapy, Alexander and Parson’s family therapy, and even Cunningham’s community BPT programs (Johnston, 1996). 231 Learned Helplessness in Practice Parents, like students with failure syndrome problems would benefit from therapy addressing self confidence in their abilities, strategies for coping with failures, and persistence in problem solving efforts under conditions of difficulty (Brophy, 1998). Cognitive retraining approaches which include attribution retraining, efficacy training, and strategy training involve more specific and elaborate suggestions and use a combination of methods (direct instruction, modeling with verbalized self instruction, thinking out loud, coaching, and scaffolded practice) (Brophy, 1998). Students with Ieamed helplessness need to learn how to cope with frustration and failure rather than a steady diet of “success” (Brophy, 1998). Ditto for parents and maybe therapists, tool. Goal Theory and PMT Practice Early, deliberate, and more systematic changes in PMT programs are required in order to affect any advantageous changes in parent goal. First and foremost, therapists need to emphasize Ieaming goals since what appears to be most salient is the individual’s perception of the goal orientation of the achievement situation (Ames & Archer, 1988). For those parents with a social orientation and excessive concern with "how their child’s behavior looks. . this is no easy task. Therapists can promote a Ieaming orientation by emphasizing what parents can Ieam from PMT activities, including collaborate work where parents help one another, and treating errors or “behavioral mishaps” as natural parts of the Ieaming process and rich material for follow up discussion (Brophy, 1998). 232 Dweck’s work suggests several implications for classroom teachers to encourage their students: 1) to adopt incremental theories rather than entity theories of abilities; 2) to set learning goals rather than performance goals; and, 3) to adopt a mastery orientation rather than a helpless orientation (Brophy, 1998). Parallel parent management training methods for establishing the first point are illustrated through the following points: setting up the therapeutic environment as a Ieaming community, focusing parent’s attention on self improvement rather than comparison with other parents, making mistakes a part of Ieaming, encouraging mastery through using successive approximations, portraying activities as opportunities to acquire and not just to display skills, . providing appropriate feedback and comments which convey confidence in reaching goals. (Brophy, 1998). Issues related to self worth protection bear special mention as this appears to be an especially salient predictor of parent training outcomes. Self worth protection in the classroom is minimized through establishing the classroom as a Ieaming community and orienting students towards Ieaming goals through models, socialization, and instruction (Brophy, 1998). Strategies by Covington (1992), Lehtinen et al. (1995), and Brophy (1998) also suggest helping the students see how the self worth protection focus limits their performance and development. Self Schemas, Possible Selves, and Practice Parents who are aschematic can become schematic through discrepant feedback and elaborating on possible selves. A viable parent study would be to examine the effect of parents teaching parents on schematicity. 233 Value Aspects and Practice As an initial step in PMT, parents require a rationale for engaging in the treatment and discussion of its benefits. While this is not new information, it tends to be relegated to therapist discretion, as well as experience. Few treatment manuals explicitly address the value aspects of motivation within PMT. The results of the current study suggest that value enhancement should be explicitly targeted at the orientation session prior to the start of PMT. Especially needed are strategies for helping parents come to value what they are Ieaming for its relevance, empowerment implications, exposure to whole aspects of life, and potential quality of life application (and not just something which needs to be done and over with). PMT may focus on building within the parents some understanding of the human condition as well as tearing down some misunderstandings and some sense that they are doing the activities engaged in something worthwhile for themselves, instead of what they should be doing. This is largely related to the parent need to “buy into” goals (as noted above) and not simply buying into strategies. (Eyberg, Edwards, Boggs, & Foote, 1998) stresses the importance of presenting a clear and concise rationale at the beginning of PMT to avoid possible attrition, loss of cases before outcome and follow-up evaluations. Furthermore, even those aspects which are already inherently valued by parents may not become as valued in PMT unless exposure is 1) scaffolded to enhance learning as well as appreciation and 2) it connects with their zone of proximal development (Brophy, 1999) 234 Summary Given the significance of the findings, all of these suggestions could culminate in an extensive comprehensive program offering Schema Mediated Parenting Training (SMEPT) (Table 53). SMEPT needs to consider that to change efficacy-based helplessness, therapists need to build in parents’ particular skills, competencies, and resiliency perceptions. These target change in some aspect of the parent. To change personal control-based helplessness, therapists need to consider some aspects of the environment (some of the more traditionally examined predictors such as SES, family structure) and some aspect of the parents’ explanatory styles (especially if pessimistic). It is also noted that self-efficacy and personal control expectations coincide in an interdependent relationship (Reeve, 1996). The implication for future practice is that although therapists (like teachers) can do little about an individual’s experience with uncontrollable outcomes and predictors (socioeconomic status, family constellation, psychosocial stressors), they can do much regarding how individuals come to interpret and understand why those outcomes occur (Reeve, 1996). 235 Table 53 Salient Motivational Aspects and Value Interventions as Potential SMEPT modules. Motivational Aspect Clinical Intervention Self Efficacy Attributions Schematicity Goals Value Verbal Persuasion Vicarious Modeling Mastery Exposure Anticipatory Stress Inoculation Attribution Re-training Self confidence Social Support Graduated Task Assignments Environmental structuring Goal Setting Goal clarification Value clarification Moral Discussion Note. Value clarification sometimes involves moral judgements. Implications For Future Research Certainly, future studies should examine the efficacy of SMEPT. Despite the extensive amount of research conducted in parent management training, the need and potential exists for further studies examining the following issues: 1. 2. The field is ripe for a meta analysis of predictors and outcomes in PMT; Further attention to the accurate prediction of the probability of drop out would assist in determining whether resources (time, etc.) should be invested. Inaccurate prediction may lead to a waste of resources on very low risk parents or, on the other hand, failure to adequately treat high risk parents; Further examination of subgroups and/or comorbidity among disruptive behavior disorders and the relationship with PMT may be warranted; The idea of matching levels of treatment to the risk level of the parent for drop out and to enhance positive outcomes aspect provides a bridge between assessment, classification, and treatment; Students with an emphasis on Ieaming goals process information using deeper level strategies while students with performance goals are more likely to use surface level memorizing (Brophy, 1998). This has implications for the generalization of treatment strategies into other areas and is another area for further research; Previous research showing increased resistance to therapists may have been related to attributional implications of therapist teaching in a didactic manner (implication parents lack knowledge and skill); more Socratic, cooperative, collaborative, and supportive may allow parents to continue to feel more self- efficacious while making the behavioral changes; One area of study which has not been sufficiently explored is who profits from single modality treatments such as PMT or from combined treatments (Kazdin, 1997). Also, unexplored is whether certain parents profit from certain types of PMT treatments as opposed to others. It has been hypothesized that certain parents and children may be a “good fit” for a particular type of parent training program (Holden et al., 1990); Clarizio et al. (1994) posed several questions related to instructional approaches and values as follows: Would you favor differ approaches for different students? For example, which approach would be better for those children who come from homes that have emphasized order, discipline, and 237 9. work? How about children from homes that valued openness, challenge, creativity, and spontaneity? Which of these approaches would be best if we want students to get the big “Picture”? (Clarizio etaL,1994,p.172x Future studies could examine evaluating/measuring parent zone of proximal development; few have considered parent actual abilities or intelligence; 10.The interplay between parent cognitions, affect, and behavior is an 11. area worthy of study (Johnston, 1996). Interventions may need to be aimed at all of these components; focus on parenting as an integration of all of these; Despite links between underachievement, language delays, reading disabilities, and CD, PMT research has rarely if ever included an academic skills enhancement program for parents (Webster-Stratton, 1993). Research is needed in programs which train parents to enhance and help children with their academics; 12. Programs in which parents who have been through training act as facilitators and teach parents just entering as parental educators are consistent with notions of enhancing self efficacy; parents may develop more confidence when models are more alike and result in better outcomes (Fox & Parroni Hennick, 1996); 13. Possible selves and latency measures more accurate than simple more like me, not like me responses, eg. Computer program to measure time to respond; takes away social desirability component. The latency of the response may indicate whether the item is readily accessible as part of their self concept and whether or not the descriptor is not a feature of their working schema. The speculation that schematic persons, respond faster to positive items and aschematic persons respond faster to negative items; (Cross & Markus, 1994) can be evaluated and used as a more precise measure of schematicity to see if it is in fact predictive of outcomes of parent training; 14.Assess parent satisfaction at multiple points during treatment and follow-up (future study)(Brestan et al., 1999). Answers to these questions may provide information to treatment planning and may extend to planning maintenance of treatment gains (Brestan et al., 1999); 15. Ruma et al. (1996) asked the question “which treatment works best for which children” and concluded that the question remains largely unanswered. With regard to parent training, it is hypothesized that a more productive question would be to change the question to “which mode of treatment works best with which parents?” Therapists likely adjust their treatment styles, i.e., strategies 238 for instruction, based on an intuitive evaluation of parents. Through interactions with parents, therapists may come to realize what parents want and to which mode of instruction they would be most responsive. It would be highly advantageous to be able to identify those parameters of successful outcomes and to adjust parent management training to capitalize on those parameters; 16.The history of Ethnotheory refers to folk perspectives on the nature of children and of parental roles in child development (Goodnow, 1995). These are described as less formal and less fully articulated, but characteristic of members in particular societies or cultures. For example, maternal beliefs about child development and parenting practices among Quebecois, Vietnamese, and Haitian mothers living in Canada varied depending upon their culture of origin (Pomerleau et al., 1991). Quebecois mothers saw children as unique competent individuals and viewed the mother’s role as important in stimulating the infant. These mother with origins to France practiced encouragement of exploratory behavior and presentation of stimulating toys and activities to their children. Vietnamese mothers, in contrast emphasized formal Ieaming activities. An important assumption of Ethnotheories is that beliefs cannot be separated from the cultural milieu (Goodnow, 1995). Ethnotheories view all members of particular cultures as possessing an underlying belief structure regarding the children’s growth and development (Goodnow, 1995). Consequently, it could be hypothesized that an Ethnotheory perspective would predict that parents from similar cultures will use similar childrearing methods; 17. Notions of collective efficacy and its influence as well as culture influence on goal orientation may be another area to study. This is somewhat related to the notion that overall classroom setting, and possibly school and even community culture may play a role in students’ goal orientations (Lemos, 1996); 18. Predictors of PMT outcomes regarding disruptive behavior need not be so domain specific; note, same predictors of negative outcomes for PMT designed to reduce risks and enhance protection against early substance use in children (Kosterman, Hawkins, Spoth, Haggerty, & Zhu, 1997); 19. Future research may see if multisystemic approaches decrease learned helplessness in parents; 20.The development of a teacher motivational schema scale (TMSS) may be of interest in predicting outcomes with children with behavior problems in the classroom and ultimately provide an aid in consultation based classroom management; 239 21. Frequent parent comments among the groups involved the lack of time to implement some of the strategies, to spend with their children, increasing demands of work, and their children’s structured extra-curricular activities. Qualitative studies have dominated the PMT field. The field is also ripe for the inclusion of some ethnographic studies to further examine some of these Issues. Final Thoughts Like some of the more complete packages integrating different theoretical traditions in mathematics (Bell & Kanevsky, 1996) and reading comprehension (Shawaker & Dembo, 1996), PMT can combine elements from motivational treatments into one comprehensive package. In fact, practice in the future creates entire possibilities for incorporating motivational modules into parent management training programs for parents of children with disruptive behavior disorders. Further experimental and new qualitative studies would be the next step to determining if enhancing parent motivation would foster more optimal outcomes. 240 APPENDIX A Parent Motivational Schema Scale (PMSS) Contents of this Appendix include the following: 1. Inter-rater Agreements on Original PMSS Items (Table A. 1) 2. Scale Items Listed by Domain (Table A. 2) 241 Table A.1 Agreement Among Independent Raters. ExpertI Student! Decision STATEMENTS Researcher Literature Judgment Surveys 1. I am confident that I can learn the material with Efficacy Efficacy Retain the other parents in education sessions. 2. Involvement in parent training will help me learn Value Value Retain how to manage my child’s behavior and help me feel better about myself. 3. Whether my child follows directions, obeys 1. Implicit Retain/ instructions, and acts appropriately or not is Pessimistic theory discuss deeply ingrained in his/her personality. It cannot 2. Implicit be changed much. 4. It is important for me to learn what is being Value Value Retain taught in these groups. 5. Making mistakes is a part of learning. Schemati- Learning Drop city goal 6. When I really try, I can get through to my child Efficacy Efficacy Retain when he or she is being most difficult. 7. I would like parent training activities that let me Perform Perform Retain show how good I am at managing behavior. goal- goal- Enhancing enhancing 8. I think that what I would Ieam in a parent Value Value Retain trainm class would be interesting. 9. If my child became disruptive and noisy in public Efficacy Efficacy Retain places (malls, church), I feel assured that I know some techniques to redirect him or her quickly. 10.I would like to show my group leader, therapist, Perform Perform Retain that I am better at managing behavior than the goal- goal- other parents. enhance enhancing 11. lwork hard to learn. Learning Learning Retain goal goal 12. By trying a different discipline method, I can Optimistic Efficacy Drop significantly affect my child’s behavior. style 13. When my child is in one of his/her Optimistic Optimistic Retain moods, there is usually something that style style set him off but it can be handled. 14. My child’s behavior is something very basic Pessimistic ImpliCit Discuss about him/her and it can’t be changed much. style theory 15. I think that what I would Ieam in a parent group Value Value Retain would be useful for me to know. 16. At group sessions, I always try to avoid being Perform Perform Retain among the poorest students/parents. goal — goal- defeatirg defeating 17. The therapist will try to make sure I Optimistic Learning Drop understand the work. style goal 18. There is really very little I can do to insure that Efficacy Efficacy Retain my child behaves appropriately. 242 19. Involvement in PMT will be helpful in improving Value Value Retain the quality of our lives. 20. I would like to show my other relatives who are Perform Perform Retain parents (my parents, siblings, cousins), that I am goal- goal- better at managing behavior than other parents. enhancing enhancing 21. I will like what I am learning in these gLoups. Value Value Retain 22. Other parents want to know how I am doing ? Perform Drop on assignments. goal- enhancing 23. The therapist will want us to try new things ? Learning Drop goal 24. At home, it is important for me to avoid looking Perform Perform Retain stupid. goal — goal- defeating defeatig 25. Involvement in PMT will be useful in helping me Value Value Retain to reach my goals. 26. Iwill feel most successful in parent training Learning Learning Retain when I learn something I didn’t know before. goal goal 27. If I could choose to have children, if I had the 7 Efficacy Drop decision to make over again, I would choose parenthood. 28. I really don’t like to make mistakes. Perform Perform Discuss goal — goal- defeating enhancing 29. My child experiences difficulties Optimistic Optimistic Retain because of his school or home style style environments which can be altered to suit his/her needs. 30. I like parent training work I will learn from even if Learning Learning Retain I make a lot of mistakes. goal goal 31. I am confident that I can make the decisions that Efficacy Efficacy Retain are made in our home. 32. The worst thing about making mistakes at home Perform Perform Retain and discussing them in groups is what other goal- goal- people may notice. defeatig defeating 33. There is not much that can be done to change Pessimistic Implicit Discuss my child’s behavioral traits (e. g. oppositional, style theory aggressive, defiant) 34. I am confident that I can have my son or Efficacy Efficacy Retain daughter listen to me and comply when I am discussing household chores. 35. The main reason I would do my parent training Learning Learning Retain homework is because I like to Ieam. goal goal 36. My child gets oppositional, hostile, or Optimistic Optimistic Retain defiant because of situational style style circumstances, yet he can be appeased with appropriate discipline. 37. I am good at controlling my child’s behavior and Efficacy Schematicit Discuss this is important to me. y 38. I will like parent training work best when it Learning Learning Retain makes me think. goal goal 39. When interacting with my child in public places, I Perform Perform Retain am concerned about making a fool of myself. goal- goal- defeating defeating 40. lwould feel successful in parent training if I did Perform Perform Retain 243 better than the other parents in the sessions. goal- goal- enhancing enhancing 41. Involvement in PMT will help me learn how to Value Value Retain manage my child’s behavior and others won’t look down on me so much, anymore. 42. When lparticipate in group activities, I am Perform Perform Discuss concerned about what my peers think about me. goal- goal- enhancing defeating 43. I work hard to get the homework assignments Learning Perform Discuss done goal goal- enhancing 44. Parents feel bad when they do not do as well as Perform Perform Discuss others. goal- goal- defeating enhancing 45. Involvement in PMT will help me learn how to Value Value Retain manage my child’s behavior and have better control and power. 46. Understanding the parent training work is more Learning Learning Retain important to me than getting evaluated (others goal goal looking at my behavior and approving). 47. I am good at managing my child’s behavior and Efficacy Schematicit Discuss this is important to me. y 48. When my child is in one of his/her moods, I can Efficacy Optimistic Discuss figure out what set him/her off, and handle the style sfiuafion. 49. My child’s behavior is mostly biological, and a Implicit Pessimistic Discuss big part of his/her personality, and sometimes theory style we ’re lucky to get through one of his/her tempers. 50. Whenever I talk about how unsuccessful Perform Perform Retain homework assignments have been at home, I goal- goal— am most concerned about what other parents defeating defeating think about me. 51. Only a few parents will get a lot out of .7 Perform Drop/ this parent training. goal- Discuss enhancing 52. There is little to be gained from an open, Pessimistic Pessimistic Retain accepting, and supportive relationship style style with my child, especially since my child is unmanageable. 53. Involvement in PMT will be interesting and Value Value Retain possibly fun, as I will meet other parents in similar situations. 54. Whenever I talk in group sessions, lam Perform Perform Discuss occupied (I wonder) by howl am perceived by goal- goal- other parents. enhancing defeating 55. I would like what I could learn in parent training Value Value Value groups. Items in bold were dropped Items in italics needed to be further discussed to arrive at a consensus to improve content sampling for each motivational resource Note: difficulty with performance goal judgments, schematicity, and implicit theories; may be worthy of discussion in general within scale development section. 244 .0.:0000:0E:: 0. 0..:0 >E 0050 2.0.0800 0..:0 >E 55> :.:0:0:0.9 0200::00 0:0 .0:::0000 .:0:0 :0 E9: 00:.00 0: 0: 0.5. 0. 90: ... .E0E90:E0: ::0:00 0:0 2:8: 0 00: 050 00:.0 >__0.00:00 .00>0:0: 0..:0 >E 30: 0:0 850:0: >E c0920: 5:80:59 .0::0:.:5 0.::._ 0. 90E. .0EEE0: :0:E0: .80.: :0 0:0 :000.5 :00 0: >x00. 9.03 .00E.:0E00 0:0 .>:._0:00._0: 90:05. :0 t0: 0.: 0 ..00.00_o.: >.:00E 0. :0.>0:0: 0.0..:0 >5. 05:332. 200 0205.90. 0:05.08: .0000: 98.0.: :50 0: 090:.0 0: :00 :0.:.s 0E0E:9_>:0 090: :0 60:00 0.: :0 000000: 00:30:50 000:0:0:x0 0..:0 >5. .05_:_00.0 0:0::9::0 5.3 00000::0 0: :00 0: .000:0:0E:9.0 05:03.0 :0 000000: ::0:00 :0 .0.:00: ..0:0:.00::0 0:00 0..:0 >5. 00.80: 0: :00 :0: ::o E.: :00 :05 0:.50E00 2.000: 0. 90:: .00009 8&5: :0 0:0 5 0. 0..:0 >E :0:>> 0:25:52: 2.00 082280 2:25.30 ..>000_.::0 02:00:00: 0:0.0000 000:: 5 0::90: .050 0:: :Es .0:0:0E 0:: E00. :00 _ :0:: E00.::00 E0 . .co.>0:0: E0..:E00 050008 :0: >000_::0.:.00: 090:0 0.0:0000: 05000050 E0 . :0:>> 2:98 0:0 0E 0: :0:0._ .0::0000 :0 :00 >E 0>0: :00 . :05 E00::00 E0 . ..>000::0 E903 0E0: 50 5 000E 90 :0:: 80.0.000 0:: 00.0E :00 _ :0:: ::00::00 E0 . .000:E90: 080:0 0.003 _ .5000 :0>0 050E 0: :0.0.000 0:: 00: _ :. .:90..:0 0>0: 0: 080:0 0.000 . :_ .>_0:0::9::0 00>0:0: 0..:0 >E :0:: 900:. 00 :00 _ 0.5. >._0> >__09 0. 90:... .:0.>0:0: 0.0_.:0 >E :00::0 >_E00:.:0.0 :00 _ .0050E 0:._:.00.0 E9050 0 05>: >m .>_0.0.:0 :0: :0 E.: 89.09 0: 00:55.00: 0E00 30:: _ :0:: 090000 .00: . .3980 .0__0E: 0000.: 0..:0: 5 >06: 0:0 0>::20.0 0E000: 0..:0 >E :. .0E: 08: 0:: boE0E :0: :0 0.: 00090:. 0: 30: 30:0. 0.003 _ .0:: 00:0: 50:80 0 50:0 .8000. 0 00: 03 0E: :00. 0:: :0:EOE9 :0: 0.0 .0::0000 :0 :00 >E :. .:.:0::_0 :00E 0:.0: 0. 0:0 ..0 0: :0:>> .0::0000 :0 :00 >E 0: :000.5 :00 :00 _ >9: >__09 _ :0:>> .6850 :00 moLDOmmm _mCO=m>=O§ >D DOE: mEm: mmS—n— N.< 0.00... 245 .00.:0: >0E 0.:00: :0::0 :0:>> 0. 0:090 5 E0:: 05000050 0:0 0E0: :0 00x0:0.E 05:.0E :00:0 05:: :90; 0: .r .0E :00:0 E.:: 0::90: :0::0 :0:>> :00:0 00E00:00 :00E E0 . .0E0: :0 :00: 0>0: 0::0E:0.000 0:030E0: .0:0000000:0 30: :00:0 5.0: _ :0>0:0:>> .:.00>E :0 .00: 0 050E 0: :0: 00E00:00 E0 . .0000_: 0..:0: 5 0..:0 >E :53 05:00:95 :0:>> .0E :00:0 E.:: 0:00: >E :0:>> :00:0 00E00:00 E0 . .00.:.>.:00 :00:0 0:: 5 9050.00: . :0:>> .0::90: .050 >: 002090: E0 _ 30: >: C0803 _: 00.:0000 E0 . .8588 :00:0 5 5.0: _ :0>0:0:>> 850000.500: .9050 00 :03 00 00:0: 00 >0:: :0:>> 00: .00: 0::90: .0000 0::0E:0.000 :.:030E0: 0:: :00 0: 0:0: 0:03 . .0559: E90: 0.:: :0 :00 :0. 0 :00 .52, 0::90: 30: 0 >.:O .00:0:0.E 050E 0: 0:... :.:00 >__09 _ .0::0E:0.000 :0 0500 E0 . 30: 30:0. 0: E02, 0::90: :0::0 .0::90: .050 :0:: 850:0: 0500:0E :0 :0::0: E0 _ :0:: ..050000 00530 .0::90: >E: 0::90: 90 0:3 82:09 .050 >E 30:0 0: 00... 0.003 . .0::90: .050 0:: :0:: :0.>0:0: 0500:0E :0 :98: E0 _ :0:: .:0.:0._0:: .:0000. :00:0 >E 30:0 0: 00... 0.003 _ .:0.>0:0: 0500:0E :0 E0 _ 0000 30: 30:0 0E :0. :0:: 00.:.>.:00 0550:: E90: 00... 0.003 _ .0:0.0000 0:: 5 0::90: .050 0:: :0:: :98: 0.0 _ :. 0550:: E90: 5 .0:0000000 .00: 0.003 . :0:.0:m::0.:.00: 0:000 00:02:89: ..05>9::0 0:0 :0.>0:0: >E :0 050.00. 0.050: 090508 05:00 :0:: 0E 0: ::0::0:E. 90E 0. 0:03 0550:: E90: 0:: 0580:9005: 000.0:0.E :0 :0. 0 050E . :. :0>0 E9: E00. .2 0:03 0550:: E90: 00... . .:.5:: 0E 00x0E :. :0:>> :00: 0:03 0550:: E90: 00... _ .E00. 0: 00... _ 000000: 0. 0:030E0: 0550:: E90: >E 0500 E0 _ :0009 50E 0: ._. 90:0: 30:0. :.:0.0 _ 05::0E00 E00. _ :0:>> 0550:: E90: 5 .0:0000000 :00E .00: :5... 005:: 30: >:: 0: 00 0E0; :0.:0:0:: 0: :. .0003 0:: 0:0:900c0 _ 900 000.0E :0.:0:0:: 0: 5 .E00. 0: 0:0: 0:03 . .05E00. :0 t0: 0 0. 00x0:0.E 050.05. 3:8... 00:.>...:m E0E0>0.:0< 00:00 N.< 0.:0... 246 0:090 000:: 5 0:.E00. E0 _ :0:3 00... _ 0:090 000:: 5 E000: 050: 0. :0:3 E00. 0: 0E 5: E0:0:E. 0. :_ 055995 0. 000.0 0.:: 5 0:.E00. 90 03 :0:3 :0:: E.:: . .305. 0: 0E 5: 5:000 0: ___3 :00:0 0.:: 5 0:.E00. ___3 _ :0:3 :0:: E.:: _ .002. :00 :0 5.000 0:: 0:305E. 5 .250: 0: ___3 :.2: 5 E0E0>_0>:. .0.000 >E :009 0: 0E 05:5: 5 .2000 0: __.3 #5.: 5 E0E0>.0>5 .0:0.:00:.0 5:50 5 0::90: :0::0 :00E __.3 _ 00 .:0: >508: 0:0 05:00:95 0: _._3 ...5.n. 5 E0E0>_0>:. .90E>:0 .:00E 00 0E :0 :300 0.00. :.:03 05:5 0:0 530:0: 0.0..:0 >E 000:0E 0: 30: E00. 0E :.0: __.3 :2: 5 ::0E0>.0>:_ .530: 0:0 .9E00 :0::0: 0>0: 0:0 530:0: 0.0..:0 >E 000:0E 0: 30: E00. 0E :.0: .__3 .52: 5 E0E0>_0>:. .:.00>E :00:0 :95: .00: 0E :.0: 0:0 530:0: 0.0..:0 >E 000:0E 0: 30: E00. 05 :.0: ._.3 t2: 5 E0E0>.0>:. 00.0> .0E 0: E0:5:E. 0. 0.:: 0:0 530:0: 0.0..:0 >E 0500:0E :0 0000 E0 _ .0E 0: E0:5:E. 0. 0.:: 0:0 530:0: 0.0..:0 >E 0:...9E00 :0 0000 E0 . 85200 5005.0005300 3:0:00 .0>.000.000 ..0:0.:.00::0 0.0. 050:: .05.>0:0: 0.0..:0 >E 00:0:0 0: 0:00 0: :00 :0:: :00E :0: 0. 90: .r .:00E 000:0:0 0: :0::00 :. .>:__0:00:0: 550.: 5 005905 2:80 0. :0: 5 >_0:0.::9::0 0:00 0:0 .0:0.:00::05 0>0:0 .80.:0050 030:0: 0_.:0 >E :0::0:>> .:00E 000:0:0 0: :.:00 :. 0:0 :0::5: :00:0 0.00: >5> 05::0E00 0. 530:0: 0.0..:0 >5. 8.52: 50.35. .0::90::0E000:0 :0900: 0:: 0:0E0 050: 0.0>0 0: >:: . .85080 :00:0 :< .0520 050.00. 0.0>0 0: 0E 5: E0085 0. :. .0E0: :< 0.250 N.< 050:. 247 APPENDIX B Consent Forms CONSENT FORM FOR PARENT SURVEY The Group Health Centre and the College of Education at Michigan State University support the practice of protection for human subjects participating in research. A brief description of the present study is as follows: The goal of this study is to develop a new survey which can be used when working with parents in the Department of Communication Disorders as well as parents in other centres. It is necessary to establish the reliability (each question is stable) and valid (each question measures what it is supposed to measure. By completing the enclosed questionnaire, you will help me to develop a reliable and valid survey. Your participation is voluntary. Even if you agree to participate you may withdraw at any time. You have been provided with the opportunity to ask questions about the study. Should you have more questions about the research, you have been encouraged to contact Dr. P. Avery, the chair of the Research and Ethics Board, Group Health Centre (254-4139) and/or Dr. D. Wright, the chair of the University Committee on Research Involving Human Subjects (1-517-355-2180). All information will be reported in group format and your name will NOT be associated with research findings. Any identifying information will not be included, that is, your name will not be written on any forms. Principle Investigator: Anne Marie Caruso, M.A., M.B.A. School Psychology Doctoral Candidate Michigan State University Project Supervisors: Harvey Clarizio, Ph.D Department Chair, CEPSE Michigan State University You indicate your voluntary agreement to participate by completing and returning this questionnaire. 248 CONSENT FORM FOR PARTICIPATION IN RESEARCH PROJECT The Group Health Centre and the College of Education at Michigan State University support the practice of protection for human subjects participating in research. A brief description of the present study is as follows: In this study, I am interested in finding out what it is that influences what works in parent behavior management sessions. I plan on looking at 1) predictors that have already been studied by other researchers and 2) parent’s thinking about involvement. The eventual aim is to use these predictors to develop the best approach for working with parents and helping them help their children. Your participation is voluntary. Even if you agree to participate you may withdraw at any time. You have been provided with the opportunity to ask questions about the study. Should you have more questions about the research, you have been encouraged to contact Dr. P. Avery, the chair of the Research and Ethics Board, Group Health Centre (254-4139) and/or Dr. D. Wright, the chair of the University Committee on Research Involving Human Subjects (1-517-355-2180). All results will be kept strictly confidential and will be stored in your medical record file upon completion of the study. All information will be reported in group format and your name will NOT be associated with research findings. Any identifying information will not be included. Signature of Participant: Print name here: Signature of Investigator Obtaining Consent: Print name here: Signature of Witness: Print name here: Principle Investigator: Anne Marie Caruso, M.A., M.B.A. School Psychology Doctoral Candidate Michigan State University Project Supervisors: Harvey Clarizio, Ph.D Department Chair, CEPSE Michigan State University 249 Letter Requesting Permission and Algorithm of REB Process Saturday, April 10, 1999 John Harwood Chief Executive Officer Group Health Centre Sault Ste Marie, Ontario PBB 1Y5 Dear Mr. Harwood, Thank you for taking the time to speak with me about a potential summer research project. I am writing to request permission to conduct a study in the Group Health Centre. In this study, I am interested in evaluating predictors of positive outcomes in parent behavior management training. I plan on looking at two broad categories of predictors (traditionally examined predictors and parent motivational resources) within the context of three categories of outcomes (changes in child behavior, parent satisfaction, and parent engagement). The eventual aim is to develop a heuristic guideline for the selection of an instructional intervention based on predictor characteristics of clients, i.e., finding the “best match” or finding the treatment approach that will maximize the benefit for parents and children. I would like to conduct the study in four phases as detailed in an attachment. These include a 1) a scale development phase, 2) participant “recruitment” phase, 3) intervention phase, and 4) outcome evaluation phase. Thus, I am requesting permission to conduct the study in the Group Health Centre; specifically, for you to supply the setting, materials, and participants with the understanding that parents may refuse to cooperate in the study. For your information, I will be mailing a second draft of the proposal which may still undergo a few minor revisions before meeting all members of my doctoral committee’s approval. I appreciate the opportunity and know that this project would be mutually beneficial. Outcome studies have shown parent behavior management training to be among the most efficacious treatments when working with children with behavior problems. 250 If it would be helpful, I would be happy to make a brief presentation about the project to any Group Health Centre staff, you deem necessary. If you would like any other information, please feel free to contact me by phone ( 517- 8871215) or e—mail (carusan@pilot.msu.edu). Again, I appreciate your time and consideration of my request and look forward to seeing you during the Research and Ethics Meeting in May. Sincerely, Anne Marie Caruso Doctoral Candidate CEPSE Michigan State University 251 Phase One (Scale Development and Factor Analysis) A scale is being developed specifically for the purposes of this study, since no one has examined motivational resources within the context of parent therapy. To ensure that each scale item content is consistent with the definition of the predictors, the content validity of the Parent Motivation Schema Scale (PMSS) is currently being established by inter-raterjudgements (10 PhD students and the professor, in a doctoral level motivation course). The scale currently contains 58 items containing 9 aspects of motivational resources which may still further be reduced. In order to establish a) whether or not the scale items actually do measure the 9 motivational resources and b) find whether or not they cluster together into common factors (maybe the 9 motivational resources actually group into two big Ms, much like intelligence factors group into big and little Gs), the scale needs to be administered to a large sample of parents and then statistically analyzed. Based on the factor analysis, we may find other items to eliminate which may not be necessary for the purposes of the study. The PMSS will be administered to parents who bring their children to the Group Health Centre. A summer student will randomly ask parents if they would be willing to complete a survey and provide direction on the completion of the Parent Motivation Schema Scale. Estimated completion time is approximately 15-20 minutes. The administration will occur while parents wait in the physician waiting rooms for their appointments. All survey responses will be unsigned and unidentified. Similarly, the clinicians in the Communication Disorders Department will ask parents if they would be willing to complete the survey and provide direction during a regularly scheduled appointment with their child. Any parent can decline participation without concern. Later the reformulated PMSS will be used to identify the predictors of outcomes in parent training (below). Phase Two (Identifying Participants / Gathering predictor information) This phase would involve identifying parents suitable for the study. The majority of participants will be parents of children already referred to the Communication Disorders Department for a behavior evaluation. These parents are parents of children on the waiting list for consults which has always been maintained by the appointment center. Parents who have been referred for services to the Communication Disorders Department will be interviewed. The parent interviews should take approximately 1 hour and all information would be kept confidential. Note that these interviews are a standard part of the assessment evaluation process whether or not parents participate in the research project. After the interview, the research project will be 252 explained to parents and they will be asked if they would like to participate in the project. They will be assured that they will receive the same services in the department whether or not they agree to participate. If they agree to participate, they will be asked to sign a consent form for participation (see enclosed). At which point, they will be administered the Parent Motivation Schema Scale (final version). The Information from the interviews will be used to establish the parent levels on each of the traditionally examined predictors (family constellation, socio- economic status, number of psychosocial stressors, severity of child behavior, and level parent depression, if any). The information from the PMSS will be used to establish level of parent motivational resources (self efficacy, attributions, goals, implicit theories, schematicity, and value). All parents (including those who agreed to participate and those who did not) will be randomly assigned to an intervention or control group. Information from the parents who did not sign the consent will not be used in the study but they will still participate in the intervention phase. Control group parents will be offered parent management training services at the end of the intervention phase (approximately two months from the point of assessment). This is not in excess of past usual wait times for therapy services in the department. .11. Phase Three (Intervention/Treatment) For several summers, the staff from the Communication Disorders Department have conducted several summer projects, including parent training which is designed to help parents to work with their children with behavior problems. The parent management training would begin immediately upon completion of the interviews and random assignment of parents to intervention or control groups. This phase would require a larger setting, i.e., larger rooms. Possibly the Women’s Health Centre’s Conference room can easily be scheduled during non- use times on a regular weekly basis (as in past summers). Phase Four (Gathering Outcome Information) During this final phase, outcome data will be gathered at the final parent therapy session and at the one month booster session and follow up. During the final therapy session, outcome data regarding changes in child behavior will be gathered through having parents complete a post-therapy, Child Behavior Checklist (pre-therapy measure also used as part of the assessment process). Outcome data regarding parent satisfaction will be gathered through having parents complete an evaluation form about the parent training. Finally, outcome data regarding parent engagement will be gathered through collecting parent workbooks and evaluating homework completion and attendance (in hours). 253 At one month follow-up (the booster session), the CBC and the Parent evaluation forms will again be re-administered. Parent workbooks will be returned. Future Phases (Developing our own Parent Training Program) The findings from this study should have several implications for theory as well as implications for future practice and therapeutic application. For example, if it is found that parent self efficacy has a significant effect on outcomes in parent therapy, then maximum benefit would be derived by designing parent training to include strategies and techniques on increasing parent efficacy. A future study (maybe next summer) may consider examining the addition of motivational modules to treatment packages as something which would further increase the efficacy of parent training treatments. 254 APPENDIX C Decisions on Factor Dimensions Tables 0.1, C2, and 0.3 present the results of an orthogonal Nine-Factor Solution using the PMSS. The Nine-Factor solution did not easily support the factor structure of the nine subscales of the PMSS as items did not readily group into the theoretical motivational aspects (Table C.3 ). Similarly, the nonorthogonal five factor solution was not as easy to interpret and did not contain adequate reliabilities (Table 0.4). The first factor in the non-orthogonal five solution contained a mix of 17 items (10 value, 5 learning goal, 1 self defeating performance goal, and 1 self enhancing performance goal items). The second factor contained 12 items: 3 optimistic explanatory style, 5 self efficacy, 1 schematicity item, 1 implicit theory, 1 pessimistic explanatory style, and 1 self defeating performance goal. The third factor contained the following 7 items: 3 self defeating performance goal, 2 self enhancing performance goal, 1 pessimistic explanatory style, and 1 schematicity. The fourth l factor contained 2 implicit theory and 1 value item. The fifth factor comprised of one item (self defeating performance goal). Due to ease of interpretation and better reliabilities for the factor scales, it was decided to use the rotated five factor solution for the subsequent data analyses. 255 I Milk 0:0- 9 00.0 5.0 00.0 E.: 00.0 00.0 00.0 0052 80.20990 02:. 0: 00.0 00.0 00.0 5.0 5.0 00.0 00.0 00.5050: 052 Em: 0.1 0: 00.: 00.0 :0 00.0 00.0 000 :3 8.0.5: 000:0:0 5.0 00.0 00.0 00.: 5... 00.0 E. 3 9.00 00.00 :82 280 0 : 0 0 0 0 0 0 0: 0&9 :o .8532 052 Emm. :0>0m 5m 0>.n. :00”. 09:... 03... 0:0 825.5000 05:00“. 8005 00.2: 9: :o 0&2. 3.: =0 8 8:200 8:09.92 0:00.250 x055; 5: 09.55000 .50 0.:0... 256 Table 0.2 Eigenvalues, Percentages of Variance. and Cumulative Percentages for Varimax Orthogonal 9 Factor Solution % of Factor Eigenvalue Variance Cumulative % One 8.872 20.164 20.164 Two 4.945 11.238 31.401 Three 4.434 10.078 41 .480 Four 1.849 4.201 45.681 Five 1.556 3.537 49.218 Six 1.349 3.067 52.285 Seven 1.243 2.826 55.111 Eight 1.18 2.683 57.793 Nine 1.061 2.411 60.205 257 we 0.:0: 5 00:0: 0.0 m :000.5 F 0:900... 0:02 50.0- «mm: .025: 50 c: 000E 90 :0:: 0:90.500 0:: 050E :00 . :0:: E00350 E0 : .00 05.0 .000: .8539: :00E 0:: :53 E90: 0:: 9.... 05:00: 0.90 0: b: 053.0 0.003 : .8988 :00:0 :< .v: "0 5:00“. 000.0 .000: .089 8:00.: :50 0: 000:0:0 00 :00 0000:: 000:: 500.500 0E0: 5 50:00 0:: :0 0000000 0E059: 00: 0..:0 >5: .mm :0 5:00“. 000.0 «mm: .:000.0E 0:: E00: :00 : :0:: E00500 E0 : .: 000.0 6:: .E00: 0: 05: E03: .0: K 5:00“. 000.0 .000: 000.0000 0:: 5 0:90: 550 0:: :0:: 5:0: 0:0 2: 80.0000 E90: 0.: 55000000 :00: 0:09: : .00 00.0.0 3mm: .0EEEO: :0QEO: 0:: :0 000 :0095 :00 0: >50: 9. 03 00E::0E00 0:0 5.500090: 5.50:: :0 :.:0Q 0.5 0 0:0 50:00:03 >.:00E 0: 530:0: 0.00:0 >3. .3. 000.0 «00.9.: .:00E 00 0E :0 0300 0:00: :.:03 9050 0:0 530:0: 0.05:0 >E 05:0: 0: 30: E00: 0E 90: :.:3 80.0000 00.0090: 0.: E0E0>5>E .00 500.0 «mm: 50:85:50 09.020: 0..:0 >E :0:: 900:: 0: 00 :00 : 05:: b9. 0.: 99:. .0: 000.0 3mm: .0.:000020E00 0.: 0:50 >E 002:0 50.5050 .0....5 >E £3. 9:80:09 02:59:00 0:0 00.30000 .0000 :0 E9: 00500 0: o: 0::... 0.. 90E 0:. :0 5:00". 05000.. 5:00 H. E0:. .000uz. wm5.n. 0:: 5: :0.:0_0w 5:00:052 5:000:00 x0E:0> 5: 005000.. 5:00“. 0:0 E0:_ :0 >5EEOw 0.0 030.. 258 Table 0.4 P Descriptives for Non-orthogonal Five-Factor Solution Factor Descriptives One Two Three Four Five Number of Items 17 12 7 3 1 Scale Mean 59.35 34.63 13.22 5.41 2.17 Standard Deviation 11.44 4.61 3.91 2.01 1.24 Item Alpha Reliabilities 0.91 0.58 0.68 0.42 na Standardized Alphas 0.92 0.58 0.70 0.44 na 259 APPENDIX D Logistic Regression Results Using Factor Scales in Lieu of Subscales The factor analysis results did not support the subscales. From the previous analyses, it appears that the factor analysis does not suggest the same dimensions as the nine motivational aspect subscales. Instead there appears to be a more collapsed structure of five dimensions. Thus, logistic regression results using the factor scales instead of the subscales are presented in Table D1 Humphries, llgren, McGrath, & Montanelli (1969) present information regarding capitalizing on chance in factor analytic research. It has been argued that purification of scales sometimes reduces the predictive value of the scale. A summary of the correlations between individual items of the PMSS and the child behavior change, parent satisfaction, and parent engagement (drop out) outcomes is presented in Table D2. It is apparent that only a select number of items (2 value, 3 self efficacy, 1 learning goal, 2 self defeating performance goals, 1 optimistic explanatory style, 1 schematicity, and 1 pessimistic explanatory style) are significantly correlated with the outcomes of the current study. 260 28.0 83 08.0 00.0 30.9 085 5:95 a :8 000.0 80.0 80.0 000.0- 808800.80 880850 80.0 08.0 08.0 000.0 83 8838.280 8:005th 82. 82.0 80.0 80.0 000.0 22080 .8898: .0595 :80 08.0 80.0 03.0 83- 80> 02503 8890 800 .90 0.02, 080 m0 900 .98“. 0900m 5:00“: 0503 :00 005 .520 0002090 0200900”: 00053 _..D 030... 261 262 11 1'10: . -... . 83 83- 33 383- 83- 83- 83- 283 83 m0 .8 :3 83 83 83 83- :3 83 :3 38.0 03 .8 83- 203- 083- 83- 8 3 083 0:3 80.: 80.0 000 .8 83 S3- 83- 33- 83 83- 083- 38.: 83 03 .8 83- 83 83. 0 S3 83- 83- 83- 83 80.0 _m> 30 83 20.83 93 .83 R3 83 83 08.: 80.0 000 .2 83 83 083 0: 3 t 3- 83 83 :.83 80.0 _0> 3: 83 93- 33. 83- 83 N83- 83 3.: 08.: won. .t 20 3 0: 3 £3- 83 83- 9 3- 83- 8 Z 83 _m> .0: 08.9 83- 38.9 083- 3.83 83 .83 83 80.: m0 .8 :83. E 3 83- 83. : : 3- .23. 83- 08.: N2 3 000 .3 83 .383 203 83 80.9 83- 83- 083 83 _a> .2 83. 083- 83 08.? 83 33 083 08.: 08.: as. .8 83. .83 83. 083 83- 83 083. 83 «5.0 000.: 83 083- 02.? 83. 083. .38.? .083. 83 28.0 04 .2 83 93- 80.? R3- 83 83. 83- 383 08.: 00.”. 3 3: 3 83 8 3. 383- 08.9 .283- 83. 38.: 83 00 .0 83 83 083 02 3 0: 09 083 83- 08.: ~83 _0> .:- 083 83 83- 083 83 83 :33 08.: v8.0 000 .0 83 .83 83 .283 80.? 83 83 83 83 m0 .0 08.? 2.083 83 .:3 809 83- 83- 0 83 28.0 :3 .0 :3- R3- 83 83- 83 83 083 28.: 80.: as. .0 83. 083 8 3 83 83 83 83 08. F 80.0 _0> .0 83- 83 83- 083 383- 083 83 083 083 m0 .: 50 no.5 N 5:00“: :- :0:omn_ ESP 055.050wa 953.0595 .90... um :00—)— E0: .5 8.80 8:80 000 00.20 00.0000> 005030 00.0000> 025050 £05 025: .0920 029200. :0 0500.950 000 00000300 0.00006 0000.2 N0 030% 000005.50 00..-..0000v. 00.00 0E0: 000.20... 5.3 00.0.0.50 0.0 00.00..0> 0:50.00 .00 00.0 0.5.00.0.50 . 000.001 00.0: 00.0.. 000.20... 5.3 00.0.0.50 0.0 00.00..0> 0:50.00 .0 .0 000 00500 000000 000 .052 .00.00-N. .82 8.0 05 .0 00000.00.» 0. 00029.8 .. 60:00-0. _0>0_ 8.0 05 .0 0:80.000 0 00009.8 . 000.0- 05 0.0 0 0N0 0 ..N0 .0000. 050.0 00 F0.- 0N00 000.0 .05 .3... 000.0- 00N.0 0 FNO 00N.0 0P 0.0. 0 FN.0 050.0 0000 0 ..00 _0> .0v 0N0.0- 0NNO. .000 ..000. $0080 00N0 N000 905.0 50N. .. mun. .NV 0500 N000- 00N0 00 ...0 v0...0 V000 50N.0 0N v.0 ..00-N QEzmwn. .3 ..000. N 00.0 000.0. N000. 00N0- '0 r0. 50N0. 000.0 0 F0-N me .9. 0.00. 00N0 N 0.0 00 F0 00N0. 000.0- 50N.0. 050.0 000.N com .00 000.0- 50 V0. 50N0. 0VNO. 00...0. ..000- 000.0. 50N.F N000 .0.—.00 00 .. .0. 30.0 000.0. 000.0. 000.0- 0N .- .0 0 00.0- 0VN... V000 .0> .50 N000. 0 ..0. 00N0. 00N0. 000.0 0NPO 0.. ..0 00 F... 0000 00.”. .00 00...0 N000. 00N0. 0N0- 00F0. 0:0. 00...? 000... 000.N OOn. .00 000.0. 500.0 000.0 050.0 000.0 05 ..0 50 ..0 50N... V5 fiN .0> 00 00N0 N0 ..0. V000. 00 P0. 00 ..0 0NNO. .000. 505.0 000.0 wmva .00 00 P .0 000.0 N30- N 50 «000.0 000.0. 500.0 ..00... 000.N 000. .N0 030 05 ..0 N0 F0 50 5.0 000.0- 50 v0. 00 F0. 005.0 00.0 0.. ...0 000.0- .5000 «000.0 .0000 5000 N000 500.0 5000 0000 now .00 000.0 00N.0 500.0 000.0 0.00. N..0.0 0.. ..0. 000.0 53.-0 mmO .0N 0.00 03.0 00.0 00.0 0000 000.0 03.0 00N€ 0N0.0 0.. .0N .... P0 000.0 0000. 000.0.- .0000. 000.0 31.0. NN0.0 v3.0 mm .5N r000- 00...0- 30.0. 50...0- 0N00 v0N0 v0N0 300 000.0 wwaEE. .0N 0000 N30- 0v00 5 00.0 00 F .0 0N00- 0N00- 00N... 00 _..N Own. .0N 00:00 Nd 0.00..- 263 APPENDIX E Method Of Calculating Socio-Economic Status Socio—economic status (SES) was calculated according to two methods. The first involved the Hollingshead Two Factor Index and the second involved the most recent low income cutoffs from Statistics Canada. The Hollingshead Two Factor Index (Hollingshead, 1965) has set the standard and is the most widely used in most psychological research involving PMT. The Index has been criticized for being dated and obsolete in its list of occupations. However, few better alternatives are available and its most recent update, the Hollingshead Four Factor Index uses marital status in its equation. Because the Four factor index confounds one of the predictor variables, it was decided to use the Two Factor Index as a continuous variable measure of SES. To calculate SES according to the Hollingshead Index, two factors (occupation and education) are assigned pre-determined scores obtained from their respective scale positions. The two factors are multiplied by pre-determined weights. The weights for each factor were originally determined by multiple correlation techniques. Weights for each factor are as follows: Table E.1 Factors and Weights used to Calculate Two Factor Index. Factor Weight Occupation 7 Education 4 The Low Income Cutoffs (LICO) from Statistics Canada (1999) are more current, relevant to the population in the study, and provide a discrete measure of SES. Estimates of income are based on a sample of 35,000 households excluding households in the territories and on Indian reserves. Cut-offs were selected on the basis that families with incomes below the limits usually spend more than 54.7% of their income on food, shelter, and clothing, and are therefore considered to be living in straightened circumstances. LlCOs are updated based on information from the annual Survey of Household Spending to reflect changes in the average proportion of family income spent on food, shelter, and clothing. As a national average, the 1998 low income cut-off is $33,063 (rounded to the nearest dollar). Seventeen and a half per cent of the Canadian population sampled is said to be low income. 264 -1! A family with income below the cutoff for its family size and urbanization classification (size of area of residence) is considered a “low income” family. Participants were coded 0 for low income or 1 for other by consulting the following table: Table E.2 Low Income Cutoffs using Number of Persons in Family Unit and Set Size of Area of Residence (30,000 to 99,999 population) Number of Persons Low Income Cutoff 14,965 18,706 23,264 28,162 31,481 34,798 7 or more 38,117 mmAwM—s If low income cutoff fell within the survey range results, participants were coded as low income. 265 APPENDIX F Power Analysis Results for T-Tests The power of a statistical test is the probability that results will be statistically significant (Cohen, 1988) or in other words, power is the probability of rejecting the null hypothesis when the null hypothesis is false. In Table E.1, power analysis results are summarized for a two-tailed, t-test for means using alpha set at .05 and alpha set at .0056 (bonferroni adjustment), respectively. Using power equals .80 as a general rule of thumb, power in this study is obviously low. Table F.1 Post Hoc Power Analysis Results Factors to Power Analyses One Two Effect Size (d) .5 .5 (medium) Alpha .05 .0056 Sample size n1 64 64 Sample size n2 29 29 Delta 2.4614 2.4614 Critical t (101) 1.9837 2.8310 Power .6837 .361 1 266 AH _. BIBLIOGRAPHY Abikoff, H.B. , & Hecktman, L. (1996). Multimodal therapy and stimulants in the treatment of children with ADHD. In E.D. Hibbs & P.S. Jensen (Eds.), Psychosocial Treatments for Child and Adolescent Disorders. Empirically Based Strategies for Clinical Practice (pp. 341-370). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Achenbach, T. M. (1991). Manual for the Child Behavior Checklist/4-18 and 1991 profile. Burlington: University of Vermont, Department of Psychiatry. Adelman, H. S., & Taylor, L. (1997). Addressing barriers to Ieaming: Beyond school-linked services and full-service schools. American Journal of Orthpsychiatry, 67(3), 408-421. Alworth, J. (1973). Home token economy: An incentive program for children and their parents. Champaign, Illinois: Research Press. American Psychiatric Association. (1980). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (3rd ed.). Washington, DC: Author. American Psychiatric Association. (1987). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (3rd ed., revised). Washington, DC: Author. American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.) Washington, DC: Author. Ames, C., & Ames, R. (1984). Systems of student and teacher motivation: Toward a qualitative definition. Journal of Educational Psycholog)’. 76(4), 535- 556. Ames, C., & Archer, J. (1988). Achievement goals in the classroom: Students' Ieaming strategies and motivation processes. Journal of Educational Psychology, 80(3), 260-267. Ames, R. (1985). Adult motivation in the leadership role: [An attribution value analysis. Advances in Motivation and Achievement, 4, 333-369. Anastopoulos, A.D., Barkley, R.A., Sheldon, TL. (1996). Family based treatment: Psychosocial intervention for children and adolescents with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder. In E.D. Hibbs & P.S. Jensen (Eds.), Psychosocial Treatments for Child and Adolescent Disorders. Empirically Based Strategies for Clinical Practice (pp. 267-284). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. 267 Applegate, B., Lahey, B. 8., Hart, E., Biederman, J., Hynd, G. W., Barkley, R., Ollendick, T., Frick, P. J., Greenhill, L., McBumett, K., Newcom, J. H., Kerdyk, L., Garfinkel, B., Waldman, I., & Shaffer, D. (1997). Validity of the age of onset criteria for ADHD: A report from the DSM-IV field trials. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry, 36(9), 1211-1221. Association for Advanced Training in the Behavioral Sciences (1995). Research design and statistics. Westlake Village, California: AATBS. August, G. J., & Garfinkel, B. D. (1993). The nosology of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry, 32(1), 155-165. Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy, the exercise of control. New York: W.H. Freeman & Co. Bank, L., Marlowe, J.H., Reid, J.B., Patterson, G.R., & Weinrott, MR. (1991). A comparative evaluation of parent-training interventions for families of chronic delinquents. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 19(1), 15-33. Barkley, RA. (1987). Defiant children: A clinician's manual for parent training. New York: Guilford Press. Barkley, R. A. (1990). Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder: A handbook for diagnosis and treatment. New York: Guilford Press. Barkley, RA. (1992). ADHD: What do we Know? [Video]. New York: Guilford Publications, Inc. Barkley, RA. (1993). Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder in children and adults. Symposium conducted at the meeting of the New England Institute of Psychiatry, Cape Cod, Massachusetts. Barkley, RA. (1995). Taking charge of ADHD : The complete, authoritative guide for parents. New York: Gulford press. Barkley, RA. (1996). Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. In E.J. Mash & R.A. Barkley (Eds), Child Psychopathology (pp. 63-112). New York: Guilford Press. Barkley (1997a). ADHD and the nature of self control. New York: Guilford Press. Barkley, R.A. (1997b). Defiant children: A clinician's manual for assessment and parent training [2nd Edition]. New York: Guilford Press. 268 Barkley, RA (1998). Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. In E.J. Mash & R.A. Barkley (Eds.), Treatment of Childhood Disorders (2nd Ed., pp. 55-110). New York: Guilford Press. Barkley, RA. (2000). Commentary: Issues in training parents to manage children with behavior problems. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry, 39(8), 1004-1006. Beck, A.T., Steer, R.A., & Brown, G. K. (1996). BDI-II Manual. New York: The Psychological Corporation, Harcourt Brace & Company. Becker, WC. (1977). Parents are teachers. New York: Guilford Press. Bell, 0., & Kanevsky, L. (1996). Promotingpositive achievement motivation in a regular Grade 2 classroom. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Educational Research Association, New York. Bickman, L., Hefiinger, C. A., Northrup, D., Sonnichsen, S., & Schilling, S. (1998). Long term outcomes to family caregiver empowerment. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 7(3), 269-282. Biederman, J., Farone, S. S., Milberger, S., & Doyle, A. (1993). Diagnosis of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder from parent reports predict diagnoses based on teacher reports. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry, 32(2), 315- 321. Bierman, K. L., Cole, J. 0., Dodge, K. A., Greenburg, M. T., Lochman, J. E., 8 McMahon, R. J. (1996). Integrating social-skills training interventions with parent training and family focused support to prevent conduct disorder in high- risk populations. The fast track multisite demonstration project. Annals New York Academy of Sciences, 256-264. Bilbrey, J., & Bilbrey, P. (1995, July/August). Judging, trusting, and utilizing outcomes data: A survey of behavioral health care payers. Behavioral Health Care Tomorrow, 62-65. Black, M.M. & Holden, E.W. (1995). Longitudinal intervention research in children's health and development. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 24(2), 163-172. Bloomquist, M. L. (1996). Skills training for children with behavior disorders: A parent and therapist guidebook. New York: Guilford Press. Brestan, E., & Eyberg, S. M. (1998). Effective psychosocial treatments for children and adolescents with disruptive behavior disorders: 29 years, 82 studies, and 5275 kids. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 27(2), 179-188. 269 Brestan, E. V., Jacobs, J. R., Rayfield, A. D., 8 Eyberg, S. M. (1999). A consumer satisfaction measure for parent-child treatments and its relation to measures of child behavior change. Behavior Therapy, 30, 17-30. Brophy, J. (1998). Motivating students to learn. New York: McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc. Brophy, J. (1999). Toward a model of the value aspects of motivation in education: Developing appreciation for particular Ieaming domains and activities. Educational Psychologist, 34, 75-86. Brunnquell, d., Crichton, L., & Egeland, B. (1981). Maternal personality and attitudes and disturbances of child rearing. Amer. J. Orthopsychiat., 51(4), 680-691. Carlson, C. (1993). The family school link: Methodological issues in studies of family processes related to children's school competence. School Psychology Quarterly, 8(4), 264-276. Chiu, C., Dweck, C. S., Tong, J. Y., & Fu, J. H. (1997). Implicit theories and conceptions of morality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73(5), 923-940. Clarizio, HP. (1996). Conduct Disorder: Developmental considerations. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the American Psychological Association, Toronto. Clarizio, H. F., Mehrens, W. A., & Hapkiewicz, W. G. (Eds). (1994). Contemporary issues in educational psychology (6th ed.). New York: McGraw- Hill, Inc. Cochran, M., & Niegro, S. (1995). Parenting and social networks. In M. C. Bomstein (Ed.), Handbook of Parenting: Status and Social Conditions of Parenting (Vol. 3, pp. 393-418). Mahwah, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum, Associates. Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behaVioraI sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Publishers. Cowen, J. (1990). Things I have Ieamed so far. American Psychologist, 45(12), 1304-1312. Comings, D. E. (1997). Genetic aspects of childhood behavioral disorders. Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 27(3), 139-149. 270 Covington, M. (1992). Makiriglhe grade: a self-worth perspective on motivation and school reform. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Cowan, EL, 8 Hightower, AD. (1988). Alternative approaches in school based preventive intervention. In R.H. Price, E.L. Cowan, R.P. Lorion, 8 J. Ramos-McKay (Eds.), Fourteen ounces of prevention: A casebook for practitioners. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Cox, M. J., Owen, M. T., Lewis, J. M., 8 Henderson, V. K. (1989). Marriage, adult adjustment, and early parenting. Child Development, 60(1015- 1024) Crnic, K., 8 Acevedo, M. (1995). Everyday stresses and parenting. In M. H. Bomstein (Ed.), Applied and Practical Parenting (Vol. 4, ). Mayway, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Publishers. Cross, 8. E., 8 Markus, H. R. (1994). Self-schemas, possible selves, and competent performance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86(3), 432-438. Dadds, MR, 8 McHugh, TA. (1992). Social support and treatment outcome in behavioral family therapy for child conduct problems. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 60(2), 252-259. Danforth, J. S. (1999). The outcome of parent training using the behavior management flow chart with a mother and her twin boys with oppositional defiant disorder and attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Child 8 Family Behavior Therapy, 21(4), 59-80. Davidson, W.S., 8 Redner, R. (1988). The prevention of juvenile delinquency: Diversion from the juvenile justice system. In R.H. Price, E.L. Cowan, R.P. Lorion, 8 J. Ramos-McKay (Eds.), Fourteen ounces of prevention: A casebook for practitioners. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Deschler, 0.0., 8 Schumaker, JB (1994). Grouping intervention research in the larger context of schooling: A response to Pressley and Harris. Educational Psychology Review, 6(3), 215-230. Dinkmeyer, D. 8 McKay, GD. (1989). STEP: Systematic training for effective parenting. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service. Drotar, D., Stein, R.E.K., 8 Perrin, EC. (1995). Methodological issues in using the Child Behavior Checklist and its related instruments in clinical child psychology research. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 24(2), 184-192. 271 I... Dumas, J.E., 8 Wahler, R.G. (1983). Predictors of treatment outcome in parent training: Mother insularity and socioeconomic disadvantage. Behavioral Assessment, 5, 301 -31 3. Dunlap, G., 8 Childs, K.E. (1996). Intervention research in emotional and behavioral disorders: An analysis of studies from 1980-1993. Behavioral Disorders, 21(2), 125-136. Dunteman, G.H. (1989). Principal components analysis. (Vol. 69). New York: Sage Publications. Durlak, J.A., Wells, A.M., Cotton, K., 8 Johnson, S. (1995). Analysis of selected methodological issues in child psychotherapy research. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 24(2), 141-148. Dweck, C. S. (1996). Capturing the dynamic nature of personality. Journal of research in personality, 30, 348-362. Dweck, C. S., 8 Leggett, E. L. (1988). A social-cognitive approach to motivation and personality. Psychological Review, 95(2), 256-273. Dyer, K., 8 Larrson, E. V. (1997). Developing functioning communicating skills: Alternatives to severe behavior disorders. In N. N. Singh (Ed.), Prevention and Treatment of Severe Behavior Problems (pp. 121-148). Detroit: Brooks/ Cole Publishing Company. Dykman, RA, 8 Ackerman, RT. (1993). Behavioral subtypes of Attention Deficit Disorder. Exceptional Children, 60(20), 132-141. Eber, L., 8 Nelson, C. M. (1997). School-based wraparound for students with emotional and behavioral challenges. Exceptional Children, 63(4), 539-556. Eifert, G.H. (1996). More theory driven and less diagnosis based behavior therapy. J. Behav. Ther. 8 Exp. Psychiat, 27(2), 75-86. Elliot, E. S., 8 Dweck, C. S. (1988). Goals: An approach to motivation and achievement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(1), 5-12. Eyberg, S. M. (1993). Consumer satisfaction measures for assessing parent training programs. In L. VandeCreek, S. Knapp, 8 T. L. Jackson (Eds.), Innovations in Clinical Practice: A source book (Vol. 12, ). Sarasota, FL: Professional Research Press. Eyberg, SM. (1995). Jigsaws: Introduction to the special issue on methodological issues in clinical child psychology research. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 24(2), 214-226. 272 .1_—’—_ Ti" p. . Eyberg, S. M., 8 Boggs, SR. (1998). Parent-child interaction therapy for oppositional preschools. In C.E. Schaefer 8 J.M. Briesmeister (Eds.), Handbook of parent training: Parents as co-therapists for children's behavior (2nd ed., pp. 61-97). New York: Wiley. Eyberg, S. M., Edwards, 0., Boggs, S. R., 8 Foote, R. (1998). Maintaining the treatment effects of parent training: The role of booster sessions and other maintenance strategies. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 5(4), 544- 554. Faul, F., 8 Erdfelder, E. (1992). A priori, mst-hoc, and compromise power analysis for MS-DOS [Computer Program]. Bonn, FRG: Bonn University, Dep. of Psychology. Finch, M. K. (1998). Social skills training for preadolescents with problem behaviors (parent training). Unpublished Dissertation, University of Illinois at Chicago, Chicago. First, J. A., 8 Way, W. L. (1995). Parent education outcomes: Insights into transformative Ieaming. Family Relations, 44, 104-109. Ford-Gilboe, M. (1997). Family strengths, motivation, and resources as predictors of health promotion behavior in single-parent and two-parent families. Research in Nursing 8 Health, 20, 205-217. Forehand, R.L., 8 Mahon, R.J. (1981). Helping the noncompliant child: A clinician's guide to parent training. New York: Guilford Press. Forgatch, M., 8 Patterson, GR. (1990). Parents and adolescents living together. Eugene, OR: Castalia. Fomess, SR, 8 Hoagwood, K. (1993). Issues in sampling, design, and implementation of school based mental health services research. School Psychology Quarterly, 8(4), 291-300. Forness, S. R., Kavale, K. A., MacMillan, D. L., Asarnow, J. R., 8 Duncan, B. B. (1996). Early detection and prevention of emotional or behavior disorders: Developmental aspects of systems of care. Behavior Disorders, 21(3), 226-240. Foster, S.L., Martinez, Jr., CR. (1995). Ethnicity: Conceptual and methodological issues in child clinical research. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 24(2), 214-226. Fox, R.A., 8 Fox, TA (1990) STAR parenting: A positive and practical approach to effective parenting. Bellevue, WA: Star Parenting, Inc. 273 ' ““7 r‘r Fox, R. A., 8 Parroni Hennick, E. (1996). Evaluating a training program for parental educators. Psychological Reports, 79, 1143-1150. Frankel, F., 8 Simmons, III, J.Q. (1992). Parent behavioral training: why and when some parents drop out. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 21 (4), 322-330. Frick, P. J. (1998). Conduct Disorders. In T. H. Ollendick 8 M. Hersen (Eds.), Handbook of Child Psychopathology (3rd Ed., pp. 213-237). New York: Plenum Press. Geller, J., 8 Johnston, C. (1995). Predictors of mothers' responses to child noncompliance: Attributions and attitudes. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 24(3), 272-278. Gillis-Amold, R., Crase, S. J., Stockdale, D. F., 8 Shelley, M. C. I. (1998). Parenting attitudes, foster parenting attitudes, and motivations of adoptive and nonadoptive foster parent trainees. Children and Youth Services Review, 20(8), 715-732. Goldberg, W. A. (1990). Marital quality, parental personality, and spousal agreement about perceptions and expectations for children. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 36(4), 531-556. Goodman, G., 8 Poillion, M.J. (1992). ADD: Acronym for any dysfunction or difficulty. The Journal of Special Education, 26(1), 37-56. Goodnow, J. J. (1995). Parents' knowledge and expectations. In M. H. Bomstein (Ed.), Handbook of Parenting: Status and Social Conditions of Parenting (Vol. 3, pp. 305-332). Mahway, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Publishers. Goodyear, P., 8 Hynd, G.W. (1992). Attention Deficit Disorder with (ADD/H) and without (ADDNVO) Hyperactivity: Behavioral and neuropsychological differentiation. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 21(3), 273-305. Gordon, T. (1975). P.E.T., parent effectiveness training: The tested new way to raise responsible children. New York: P.H. Wyden. Greenberg, G.S., 8 Horn, W.F. (1991). Attention Deficit Disorder: Questions and answers for parents. Champaign, Illinois: Research Press. 274 Greenhill, LL. (1998). Childhood Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder: Pharmacological treatments. In P.E. Nathan 8 J.M. Gorman (Eds.), A Guide to Treatments that Work, (pp. 42-64). New York: Oxford University Press. Greene, L., Kamps, D., Wyble, J., 8 Ellis, C. (1999). Home-based consultation for parents of young children with behavioral problems. Child 8 Family Behavior Therapy, 21(2), 19-45. Hall, L. A., Gurley, D. N., Sachs, B., 8 Kryscio, R. J. (1991). Psychosocial predictors of maternal depressive symptoms, parenting attitudes, and child behavior in single-parent families. NursinLResearch, 40(4), 214-220. Hardman, M. L., Drew, C. L., 8 Egan, M. W. (1997). Human exceptionality: Society, school, and family. (Fifth Edition ed.). Boston: Allyn and Bacon. Hardman, M. L., Drew, C. L., 8 Egan, M. W. (1999). Human exceptionalitLSociety, school, and family. (Sixth Edition ed.). Boston: Allyn and Bacon. Heflinger, C. A., Bickman, L., Northrup, D., 8 Sonnichsen, S. (1997). A theory-driven intervention and evaluation to explore family caregiver empowerment. Journal of Emotional and Behavioral Disorders, 5(3), 184-191. Henggeler, S. W., Schoenwald, S. K., Borduin, C. M., Rowland, M. D., 8 Cunningham, P. B. (1998). Multisystemic Treatment of Antisocial Behavior in Children and Adolescents. New York: The Guilford Press. Henggeler, S.W. Smith, B.H., 8 Schoenwald, SK. (1994). Key theoretical and methodological issues in conducting treatment research in the juvenile system. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 23(2), 143-150. Heyman, G. D., 8 Dweck, C. S. (1998). Children's thinking about traits: Implications for judgements of the self and others. Child Development, 64(2), 391-403. Hinshaw, SP. (1994). Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder. Volume 29. Developmental clinical psychology and psychiatry. Thousand Oaks, California: Sage Publications, Inc. Hinshaw, SP. (1996). Enhancing social competence: Integrating self- management strategies with behavioral procedures for children with ADHD. In E.D. Hibbs 8 PS. Jensen (Eds.), Psychosocial Treatments for Child and Adolescent Disorders. Empirically Based Strategies for Clinical Practice (pp. 285- 310). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. 275 ‘1 Hinshaw, SP, 8 Anderson, CA. (1996). Conduct and Oppositional Defiant Disorders. In E.J. Mash 8 RA. Barkley (Eds.), Child Psychopathology (pp. 113-152). New York: Guilford Press. Hinshaw, S. R, Klein, R. G., 8 Abikoff, H. (1998). Childhood Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder: Nonpharrnacological and combination treatments. In P. E. Nathan 8 J. M. Gorman (Eds.), A Guide To Treatments that Work (pp. 26-41). New York: Oxford University Press. Hoagwood, K. (1993). Introduction: Methodological issues in school-based mental health services research. School Psychology Quarterly, 8(4), 239-240. Holden, G.W., Lavigne, V.V., 8 Cameron, AM. (1990). Probing the continuum of effectiveness in parent training: Characteristics of parents and preschoolers. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 19(1), 2-8. Holden, G. W. (1995). Parental attitudes toward childrearing. In M. E. Bomstein (Ed.), Handbook of Parenting: Status and Social Conditions of Parenting (Vol. 3, ). Mahway, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Publishers. Hollingshead, AB. (1965). Two factor index of social position. New Haven, Connecticut: Yale University Station, Department of Sociology. Hollingshead, AB, 8 Redlick, RC. (1958). Social class and mental illness. New York: John Wiley and Sons. Hong, Y., Chiu, G, Dweck, C. S., 8 Sacks. (1997). Implicit theories and evaluative processes in person cognition. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 33, 296-323. Horvath, T. ( 1981). Basic research statistics. Windsor, Ontario: University of Windsor Press. Humprhies, L. G., Montanelli, R.G. (1975). An investigation of the parallel analysis criterion for determining the number of common factors. 193-205 Hutchings, J. (1996). Evaluating a behaviorally based parent training group: Outcomes for parents, children and health visitors. Behavioral and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 24, 149-170. Johnston, C. (1996). Addressing parent cognitions in interventions with families of disruptive children. In K. S. Dobson 8 K. D. Craig (Eds.), Advances in Cognitive Behavioral Therapy (pp. 193-208). Thousand, Oaks: Sage Publications: Sage Publications. 276 Kaiser, A. P., 8 Hester, P. P. (1997). Prevention of conduct disorder through early intervention: A social communicative perspective. Behavioral Disorders, 2(3), 117-130. Kanfer, F. H. (1996). Motivation and emotion in behavior therapy. In K. S. Dobson 8 K. D. Craig (Eds.), Advances in Behavioral Therapy (pp. 1-26). Thousand, Oaks: Sage Publications. Kanigsberg, J.S., 8 Levant, RF. (1988). Parental attitudes and children's self-concept and behavior following parents' participation in parent training groups. Journal of Community Psychology, 16, 152-160. Kazdin, A.E. (1987). Treatment of antisocial behavior in children: Current status and future directions. Psychological Bulletin, 102(2), 187-203. Kazdin, A.E. (1996). Problem solving and parent management in treating aggressive and antisocial behavior. In E.D. Hibbs 8 PS. Jensen (Eds.), Psychosocial Treatments for Child and Adolescent Disorders. Empirically Based Strategies for Clinical Practice (pp. 377-408). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Kazdin, A. E. (1997a). Parent management training: Evidence, outcomes, and issues. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 65(3), 453-463. Kazdin, A.E. (1997b). Practitioner review: Psychosocial treatments for Conduct Disorder in children. J. Child Psychol. Psychiat., 38(2), 161-178. Kazdin, A. (1998). Psychosocial treatments for Conduct Disorder in children. In P. E. Nathan 8 J. M. Gorman (Eds.), A Guide to Treatments That Work (pp. 65-89). New York: Oxford University Press. Kazdin, A. E., Holland, L., 8 Crowley, M. (1997). Family experience of barriers to treatment and premature termination from child therapy. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psycholfly, 65(3), 453-463. Kazdin, A. E., 8 Wassell, G. (1999). Barriers to treatment participation and therapeutic change among children referred for Conduct Disorder. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 28(2), 160-172. Kazdin, A. E., 8 Weisz, J. R. (1998). Identifying and developing empirically supported child and adolescent treatments. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 66(1), 19-36. Koegel, R. L., Bimbela, A., 8 Schreibman, L. (1996). Collateral effects of parent training on family interactions. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 26(3), 347-358. 277 Kim, Jae-On, 8 Mueller, CW. (1978). Introduction to factor analysis. (Vol. 13). New York: Sage Publications. Kim, Jae-On, 8 Mueller, CW. (1978). Factor analysis: Statistical methods and practical issues. (Vol. 14). New York: Sage Publications. Kohn, M. L. (1969). Class and conformity: A study in values. Homewood, III.: Dorsey Press. Kosterman, R., Hawkins, J. D., Spoth, R., Haggerty, K. P., 8 Zhu, K. (1997). Effects of a preventive parent-training intervention on observed family interactions: Proximal outcomes from preparing for the drug free years. Journal of Community Psychology, 25(4), 337-352. Lehtinen, E., Vauras, M., Salonen, P., Olkinuora, E., 8 Kinnunen, R. (1995). Long-term development of Ieaming activity: Motivational, cognitive, and social interaction. Educational Psychologist, 30, 21-35. Lemos, M. S. (1996). Students' and teachers' goals in the classroom. Learning and Instruction, 6(2), 151-171. Lerner, J. 8 Lerner, S. (1991). Attention Deficit Disorder: Issues and questions. Focus on Exceptional Children, 24(3), 6. Levine, MS. (1977). Canonical analysis and factor comparison. (Vol. 6). New York: Sage Publications. Levy, S. R., Stroessner, S. J., 8 Dweck, C. S. (1998). Stereotype formation and endorsement: The role of implicit theories. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(6), 1421 -1436. Lisi, M.-De., 8 Sigel, I. E. (1995). Parental Beliefs. In M. H. Bomstein (Ed.), Handbook of Parenting: Status and Social Conditions of Parenting (Vol. 3, pp. 333-349). Mahway, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Publishers. Loeber, R., 8 Stouthamer-Loeber, M. (1998). Development of juvenile aggression and violence: Some common misconceptions and controversies. American Psychologist, 53(2), 242-259. Maehr, M. L., 8 Pintrich, P. R. (Eds.). (1999). Advances in Motivation and Achievement. (Vol. 11). Stamford, CT: JAI Press Inc. Markus, H., 8 Nurius, P. (1986). Possible selves. American Psychologist, 41, 954-969. 278 Marx, B.P., 8 Gross, AM. (1998). Behavioral treatment. In T.H. Ollendick 8 M. Hersen (Eds.), Handbook of Child Psychopathology (3rd Ed., pp. 581-602). New York: Plenum Press. Matson, J. L., 8 Duncan, D. (1997). Aggression. In N. N. Singh (Ed.), Prevention and Treatment of Severe Behavior Problems (pp. 217-236). Detroit: Brooks/Cole Publishing Company. McBumett, K., Lahey, B. B., 8 Pfiffner, L. J. (1993). Diagnosis of Attention Deficit Disorders in DSM-IV: Scientific basis and implications for education. Exceptional Children, 60(2), 108-117. McConaughy, S. H., Kay, P. J., 8 Fitzgerald, M. (1998). Preventing SED through parent-teacher action research and social skills instruction: First-year outcomes. Journal of Emotional and Behavioral Disorders, 6(2), 81-93. McMahon, R.J., 8 Wells, KC. (1998). Conduct Problems. In E.J. Mash 8 RA. Barkley (Eds.), Treatment of Childhood Disorders (2nd Ed., pp. 111-210). New York: Guilford Press. Meece, J. L., 8 Holt, K. (1993). A pattern analysis of students' achievement goals. Journal of Educational Psychology. 85(4), 582-590. Midgley, C., Arunkumar, R., 8 Urdan, T. C. (1996). "If I don't do well tomorrow, there's a reason": Predictors of Adolescents' use of academic self- handicapping strategies. Journal of Educational Psychology, 88(3), 432-434. Midgley, C., Feldlaufer, H., 8 Eccles, J. S. (1989). Change in teacher efficacy and student self- and task-related beliefs in mathematics during the transition to junior high school. Journal of Educational Psychology, 81(2), 247- 258. Moreland, J.R., Schwebel, A.I., Beck, 8., 8 Wells, R. (1982). Parents as therapists: A review of behavior therapy parent training literature - 1975-1981. Behavior Modification, 6, 250-276. Morgan, A. E., Hynd, G. W., Riccio, C. A., 8 Hall, J. (1996). Validity of the DSM-IV ADHD predominantly inattentive and combined types: Relationship to previous DSM diagnoses/ subtype differences. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry, 35(3), 325-333. Murray, BA, 8 Myers, MA. (1998). Conduct Disorders and the Special- Education Trap. The Education Digest, 48-54. 279 Nathan, PE, 8 Gorman, J.M. (1998). Summary of treatments that work. In P.E. Nathan 8 J.M. Gorman (Eds.), A Guide to Treatments that Work, (pp. xix- xxx). New York: Oxford University Press. Newby, T.J., Stepich, D.A., Lehman, J.D., 8 Russell, JD. (1996). Instructional technology for teaching and Ieaming. Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey: Prentice Hall, Inc. Nicholls, J. G. (1984). Achievement motivation; Conceptions of ability, subjective experience, task choice, and performance. Psychological Review, 91, 328-346. Pajares, F. (1996). Self-efficacy beliefs in academic settings. Review of Educational Research, 66(4), 543-578. Patterson, GR, 8 Forgatch, MS. (1987). Parents and adolescents living together--Part I: The basics. Eugene, OR: Castalia. Pelham, W. (1996). ADHD in children, adolescents, and adults: Recent advances. Paper presented at the Meeting of the New England Institute of Psychiatry, Cape Cod, Massachusetts. Pelham, W., 8 Hoza, B. (1996). Intensive treatment: Summer treatment program for children with ADHD. In E.D. Hibbs 8 PS. Jensen (Eds.), Psychosocial Treatments for Child and Adolescent Disorders. Empirically Based Strategies for Clinical Practice (pp. 311-340). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Peterson, L., 8 Bell-Dolan, D. (1995). Treatment outcome research in child psychology: Realistic coping with the "Ten commandments of Methodology". Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 24(2), 149-162. Phelan, T. (1995). 1-2-3 Magic: Effective discipline for children 2-12 (Rev. 2nd ed.). Glen Ellyn, Illinois: Child Management Inc. Pintrich, P. R., 8 De Groot, E. V. (1990). Motivational and self-regulated learning components of classroom academic performance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82(1), 33-40. Pomerleau, A., Malcuit, G., 8 Sabatier, C. (1991). Child-rearing practices and parental beliefs in three cultural groups of Montreal: Quebecois, Vietnamese, Haitian. In M.H. Bomstein (Ed.), Cultural approaches to parenting (pp. 45-68). Hilsdale, WJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. 280 Porter, AC (1988). Comparative experiments in educational research. In R. M. Jaeger (Ed.), Complementary methods for research in education (pp. 391- 414). Washington, DC: American Educational Research Association. Power, T. J., 8 DuPaul, G. J. (1996). Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder: The re-emergence of subtypes. School Psychology Review, 25(3), 284- 296. Pressley, M., 8 Harris, KR. (1994). Increasing the quality of educational intervention research. Educational Psychology Review, 6(3), 191-208. Price, R., Cowen, E. L., Lorion, R. P., 8 Ramos-McKay, J. (1989). The search for effective prevention programs: What we Ieamed along the way. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 59(1), 49-58. Prinz, R. J., 8 Miller, G. E. (1994). Family-based treatment for childhood antisocial behavior: Experimental influences on dropout and engagement. Journal of Consultingand Clinical Psychology, 62, 645-650. Ramey, C. T., Bryant, D.M., Campbell, F.A., Sparling, J.J., 8 Wasik, B.H. (1988). In R.H. Price, E.L. Cowan, R.P. Lorion, 8 J. Ramos-McKay (Eds.), Fourteen ounces of prevention: A casebook for practitioners. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Reeve, J. (1996). Motivating others: Nurturinginner motivational resources. Boston: Allyn and Bacon. Rencher, A. C. (1995). Methods of multivariate analysis. New York: John Wiley 8 Sons, Inc. Ruma, P.R., Burke, R.V., 8 Thompson, R.W. (1996). Group parent training: Is it effective for children of all ages? Behavior Therapy, 27, 159-169. Rogers Wiese, MR. (1992). A critical review of parent training research. Psychology in the Schools, 29, 229-236. Rogers Wiese, MR, 8 Kramer, J.J. (1988). Parent training research: An analysis of the empirical literature 1975-1985. Psychology in the Schools, 25, 325-330. Shapiro, ES. (1987). Intervention research methodology in school psychology. School Psychology Review, 16(3), 290-305. Shawaker, T., 8 Dembo, M. (1996). The effects of efficacy buildinj instruction on the use of Ieaming strategies. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Educational Research Association, New York. 281 Shulman, LS. (1988). Disciplines of inquiry in education: An overview. In R. M. Jaeger (Ed.), Complementary methods for research in education (pp. 4- 17). Washington, DC: American Educational Research Association. Skaalvik, E. M. (1997). Self-enhancing and self-defeating ego orientation: Relations with task and avoidance orientation, achievement, self-perceptions, and anxiety. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89(1), 71-81. Spitzer, A., Webster-Stratton, C., 8 Hollinsworth, T. (1991). Coping with conduct problem children: Parents gaining knowledge and control. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology. 20(4), 413-427. Stahl, N., 8 Clarizio, H. (1999). Conduct disorder and comorbidity. Psychology in the Schools, 36(1), 41-49. Statistics Canada (1999, December). Low income cut-offs (LICOS). Available http:/l www.statcan.ca/cgi-bin/sear... TION = Highlight 8 ITEM = 1 8 DAILY = 990414. Stipek, 0., Salmon, J. M., Giwin, K. B., 8 Kazemi, E. (1998). The value (and convergence) of practices suggested by motivation research and promoted by mathematics education reformers. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 29, 465-488. Strayhorn, J. M., Strain, P. S., 8 Walker, H. M. (1993). The case for interaction skill training in the context of tutoring as a preventive mental health intervention in schools. Behavioral Disorders, 19(1), 11-26. Strayhorn, J. M., 8 Weidman, C. S. (1991). Follow up one year after parent-child interaction training: Effects on behavior of preschool children. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry, 30(1), 138-143. Tannock, R. (1998). Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD): Understanding and intervention. Paper presented at the 51st Annual OPA Convention, Toronto, Ontario. Tolan, P., 8 Guerra, N. (1995). What works in reducing adolescent violence: An empirical review of the field. Boulder, Co: Center for the Study and Prevention of Violence. Thompson, B. (1984). Canonical correlation analysis: Uses and interpretation. (Vol. 47). New York: Sage Publications. 282 Thompson, R. W., Ruma, P. r., Schuchmann, L. F., 8 Burke, R. v. (1996). A cost-effectiveness evaluation of parent training. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 5(4), 415-429. Tucker, 8., 8 Gross, D., (1997). Behavioral parent training: An intervention strategy for guiding parents of young children. The Journal of Perinatal Education, 6, 35-44. Tucker, 8., Gross, D., Fogg, L., Delaney, K., 8 Lapporte, R. (1998). The long term efficacy of a behavioral parent training intervention for families with 2- year-olds. Research in Nursing 8 Health, 21, 199-210. US. Bureau of the Census (1994). Statistical abstracts of the United States: 1994 (114th ed.). Washington, DC: Author. Vuchinich, S., Wood, 8., 8 Angelelli, J. (1996). Coalitions and family problem solving in the psychosocial treatment of preadolescents. In E.D. Hibbs 8 PS. Jensen (Eds.), Psychosocial Treatments for Child and Adolescent Disorders. Empirically Based Strategies for Clinical Practice (pp. 497-520). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Webster-Stratton, C. (1985). Predictors of treatment outcome in parent training for conduct disordered children. Behavior Therapy, 16, 223-243. Webster-Stratton, C. (1990). Long-term follow-up of families with young conduct problem children: From preschool to grade school. Journal of Clinical Child Psycholog, 19(2), 144-149. Webster-Stratton, C. (1991). Annotation: Strategies for helping families with Conduct Disordered children. J. Child Psychol. Psychiat., 32(7), 1047-1062. Webster-Stratton, C. (1992). The parents and children videotape series: Programs 1-10. Seattle, Washington: Seth Enterprises. Webster-Stratton, C. (1993). Strategies for helping early school-aged children with oppositional defiant and conduct disorders: The importance of home-school partnerships. School Psychology Review, 22(3), 437-457. Webster-Stratton, C. (1996a). Early-onset conduct problems: Does gender make a difference? Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 64(3), 540- 551. Webster-Stratton, C. (1996). Early intervention with videotape modeling: Programs for families of children with Oppositional Defiant Disorder or Conduct Disorder. In E.D. Hibbs 8 PS. Jensen (Eds.), Psychosocial Treatments for Child 283 and Adolescent Disorders. Empirically Based Strategies for Clinical Practice (pp. 435-474). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Webster-Stratton, C. (1998). Preventing conduct problems in Head Start children: Strengthening parenting competencies. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 66(5), 715-730. Webster-Stratton, C., 8 Hammond, M. (1990). Predictors of treatment outcome in parent training for families with conduct problem children. Behavior Therapy, 21 , 319-337. Webster-Stratton, C. 8 Hammond, M. (1997). Treating children with early- onset conduct problems: A comparison of child and parent training interventions. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 65(1), 93-109. Webster-Stratton, C., 8 Hebert, M. (1993). "What really happens in parent training?". Behavior Modification, 17(4), 407-456. Webster-Stratton, C., Kolpacoff, M., 8 Hollinsworth, T. (1995). Self- administered videotape therapy for families with conduct-problem childrenl Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 57, 550-553. Webster-Stratton, C., 8 Spritzer, A. (1996). Parenting a young child wit conduct problems. In T.H. Ollendick 8 R. J. Prinz (Eds.), Advances in clinical child psychology (Vol. 18, pp. 1-62). Weiner, B. (1979). A theory of motivation for some classroom experiences. Journal of Educational Psychology, 71(1), 3-25. Wells, KC. (1993). Parent trainiananual for oppositional children. Symposium conducted in Toronto, Ontario. Whalen, C.K., 8 Henker, B. (1998). Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorders. In T. H. Ollendick 8 M. Hersen (Eds.), Handbook of Child Psychopathology (3rd Ed., pp. 181-211). New York: Plenum Press. Wierzbicki, M., 8 Pekarik, G. (1993). A meta-analysis of psychotherapy dropout. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 24(2), 190-195. Wolery, M., 8 Winterling, V. (1997). Curricular approaches to controlling severe behavior problems. In N. N. Singh (Ed.), Prevention and Treatment of Severe Behavior Problems (pp. 217-236). Detroit: Brook/Cole Publishing Company. 284 Wood, W. I., 8 Baker, J. A. (1999). Preferences for parent education programs among low socioeconomic status, culturally diverse parents. Psychology in the Schools, 36(3), 239-247. 285