. 9...}...3.“ «his? manfigwfio 4 afifim fl. 5.. p353 ‘. J ”a .d-lsz“ . , v...n.~..wmm...afi .5....G..$ . . _ . yawn: v. A" . . . I‘- ~ ’ :2... 3.1.. , E and ‘ , .. 1.. 4 h. _ a... . . .. . L a, . , V V . .2. “ 2.99.63...» 5 w», 1...? .idntal.1.r.a?i.slpfi 11.14:. a: . . . . . :rei... . .n .. J . . z . , . ., 4 . u ‘ .w ‘ . . V , ”m... q u%a’t ~ , . . . , {1-K $01! gift". 4%... . 3.”. a has} . 5.8.5:; .351 to .wflmmfiw Jwafimpfiafam muukuhuhér. ‘mxuupfik . .m. . ”run...“ 62.3051)? iv"... . .; . . n . “$5555.. Raw. 1":5 V ‘r. a 1 . .5356 A . ‘ W: 3232!. 195...»: .. . [3119.4; .5 . h. 'l 5“! I\‘2. noun 1 ‘ .ufi . WW4 k3,. . '31.... all . . 5 “Nu. . . ‘ ”1.15.1. . (A z}- ‘ mm? \ $.51... In“ 9 . ‘ z r . .. .. ... . 33.5.5: gafifififii at {inimmum 5.5 . n .: at... .\:.I’....\.u‘ t (935 41:11.. 73:"171... .f- .1 V!!!’ ‘13:}... infifiirlr .II I. I: .91 .LR I. Ly .t. s :2. .. , _ . . . . Fumzrakwwfiit... pm. . . . , V . : . . «3.7%. ”a“ . . . . . . _ ‘ , .. ‘ a? 4 . . _ THESIS 2 2600 lllilllllllllllllllllllllllllllllllllilll 3 1293 02106 1647 LIBRARY Michigan State University This is to certify that the dissertation entitled VERBAL MEDDRY PERFORMANCE IN ABLE OLDER ADULTS: IMPACT OF MEMORY, ATTENTION, AND RELAXATION TRAINING presented by Michelle K. Daugherty has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for Ph. D. degree in 233“; ho] 081‘ mm M Major professor Date 10/ 15/ 99 MSU is an Affirmative Action/Equal Opportunity Institution 0- 12771 PLACE IN RErURN BOX to remove this checkout from your record. TO AVOID FINES return on or before date due. MAY BE RECALLED with earlier due date if requested. DATE DUE DATE DUE DATE DUE 11/00 Walls-p.14 VERBAL MEMORY PERFORMANCE IN ABLE OLDER ADULTS: IMPACT OF MEMORY, ATTENTION, AND RELAXATION TRAINING By Michelle K. Daugherty A DISSERTATION Submitted to ‘ Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Psychology 2000 ABSTRACT VERBAL MEMORY PERFORMANCE IN ABLE OLDER ADULTS: IMPACT OF MEMORY, ATTENTION, AND RELAXATION TRAINING BY Michelle K. Daugherty This study examined the effects of combined memory and attention training, combined memory and relaxation training, and no training on the verbal memory abilities of able older adults. Participants (N = 84) for this study were healthy, community-dwelling older adults who were offered two mood and memory assessments and seven memory training workshops. Verbal memory abilities were evaluated with the California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT) and the Logical Memory stories from the Wechsler Memory Scale (WMS). In contrast to findings from previous studies, most of which did not use control groups, all predictions of greater verbal memory effects for memory/attention training vs. memory/relaxation training and for both types of training vs. no training were not supported. However, post-hoe analyses indicated that participants who received memory and attention training performed better on a delayed recall task from the CVLT than participants who received memory and relaxation training. Findings also revealed that participants performed better on a task of recognition memory than on a task of unstructured recall and that their verbal memory abilities improved after being presented with cues. Implications of these findings are discussed. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS I wish to sincerely thank Drs. Dozier Thornton, Ray Frankmann, and Joel Nigg for their input and support as committee members. A special thanks is due to Joel Nigg for sharing his knowledge of statistics and data analyses. I also want to extend my deepest appreciation to Dr. Norman Abeles for his valuable guidance and essential contributions throughout my work on this dissertation. Furthermore, I am grateful for the numerous opportunities with which he has provided me that have enabled me to greatly expand my clinical experience and have helped shape my future career. In addition, I want to thank my family for their unconditional love and support. I especially want to thank my husband, Doug, and my daughter, Madeline, for being the joy in my life. Watching Madeline grow over the past 16 months has provided me with a sense of amazement and clarity of purpose that is unlike anything I previously had experienced. iii TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES .................................................................................. vi INTRODUCTION ................................................................................... 1 Attention in Older Adults ................................................................. 2 Definitions of Attention .......................................................... 2 Theories of the Aging of Attention ............................................ 6 Memory in Older Adults ................................................................ 14 Models of Memory .............................................................. 15 Encoding, Storage, and Retrieval .......................................... 18 Verbal Memory .................................................................. 21 The Relationship between Depression and Memory .................. 24 Improvement in Memory after Cognitive Retraining in Older Adults ........ 28 The Method of Loci ............................................................. 28 The Face-Name Mnemonic .................................................. 31 Attention/Concentration Training ........................................... 35 General Summary and Hypotheses ................................................ 39 Hypotheses ....................................................................... 40 METHOD ........................................................................................... 44 Participants ............................................................................... 44 Measures .................................................................................. 46 Beck Depression Inventory (BDI) ........................................... 46 Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS) .......................................... 46 Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) .................................. 47 California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT) ................................... 48 Wechsler Memory Scale (WMS) - Logical Memory ................... 49 Procedure ................................................................................. 50 RESULTS .......................................................................................... 54 Hypothesis 1 .............................................................................. 61 Hypothesis 2 .............................................................................. 62 Hypothesis 3 .............................................................................. 63 Hypothesis 4 .............................................................................. 64 Hypothesis 5 .............................................................................. 66 DISCUSSION ...................................................................................... 70 APPENDIX A Beck Depression Inventory (Beck et al., 1961) .................................. 86 APPENDIX B Geriatric Depression Scale (Yesavage et al., 1983) ............................ 88 iv Table of Contents (Cont’d) APPENDIX C Mini-Mental State Examination (Folstein et al., 1975) .......................... 90 APPENDIX D California Verbal Learning Test (Delis et al., 1987) ............................. 91 APPENDIX E Wechsler Memory Scale — Logical Memory (Wechsler, 1955) ............... 98 REFERENCES .................................................................................. 101 Table 1: Table 2: Table 3: Table 4: Table 5: LIST OF TABLES Demographic Characteristics of Memory and Attention (M+A) Training (Group A), Memory and Relaxation (M+R) Training (Group B), and Control Groups ............................................. 56 Correlations among Pretest Dependent Variables ..................... 58 Correlations Among Dependent Measures and Pretest Dependent Variables .......................................................... 59 Test-Retest Reliabilities of Dependent Variables (Pretest to Posttest) ........................................................................... 6O Univariate Analyses of Variance for Dependent Variables (with BDI as a Covariate) ............................................................ 68 vi INTRODUCTION There are now more than 33 million Americans who are age 65 and over; furthermore, the fastest growing group in the population consists of individuals over the age of 85 (Doolittle & Wiggins, 1993). Currently, for persons who are 65 years of age, the mean life expectancy is approximately 19 years for females and 15 years for males (LaRue, 1992). Thus, as the population of elderly adults increases, more research examining the abilities of these individuals is needed. Although many older adults are healthy, it is generally accepted that certain cognitive functions decrease with increasing age. In particular, findings in the literature provide support for a decrease in certain memory abilities of older adults (Neely & Backman, 1995). Older adults frequently complain about verbal memory difficulties, such as remembering names, lists, and conversations (Yesavage, Rose, & Bower, 1983). It has also been suggested that limited or deficient attentional abilities of elderly persons may contribute to the memory deficits these individuals experience (Yesavage & Jacob, 1984). Research has shown that healthy older adults can enhance their memory functioning by Ieaming certain memory training techniques (Yesavage & Rose, 1984b), although findings regarding the effects of combining memory training with an attention training component are very limited. As it is important to continue to develop and expand methods designed to improve memory functioning in old age, this study investigated the effects of memory and attention training techniques on the verbal memory abilities of healthy older adults. Attention in Older Adults Attention has been a major topic in research since the beginning of experimental psychology in the late 18005 (McDowd & Birren, 1990). It is defined as being "responsible for selectively preparing for, maintaining the preparation for, and processing certain aspects of experience. Attention is also responsible for the coordination of multiple simultaneous tasks" (Hartley, 1992, p.5). However, attention in older adults did not become a focus of research pursuit until more recently (McDowd & Birren, 1990). This lack of emphasis on exploring the role of attention in cognitive functioning in the elderly is striking in light of its essential role in one performing cognitive tasks to the best of one's ability (LaRue, 1992; McDowd & Birren, 1990). Hence, if the ability to attend to stimuli is impaired, many other cognitive abilities, such as memory, likewise may be adversely affected. Learning and memory capacities have been shown to exhibit deficits due to attentional dysfunction; thus, one factor that may contribute to the reported age—decrements in memory in older adults is a decrease in attentional abilities (Craik & Simon, 1980). However, studies assessing attention in older adults often have yielded mixed results which has lead to some confusion in the literature (LaRue, 1992). Definitions of Attention A review of the literature revealed various definitions of attention. Attention as a general ability has been defined as a "global limited capacity to support cognitive processing" (Plude & Hoyer, 1985, p.57). Attention is thought "to be responsible for the transfer of information from the sensory memories to short-term/working memory" and essential in any conscious or semi-conscious cognitive processes (Ashcraft, 1989, p.64). According to a model of attention by Pribram and McGuinness, attention consists of arousal ("a phasic short-lived and reflex response to input"), activation ("a tonic long-lasting and involuntary readiness to respond"), and effort (which coordinates arousal and activation and leads to voluntary control) (McGuinness & Pribram, 1980, p.99). Most theories of attention assume that the amount of information to which one can attend is limited and that the maximum limit determines the number of effortful or controlled processes that can occur concurrently (Ashcraft, 1989). According to LaRue (1992), commonly examined categories of attention include sustained attention/concentration, divided attention, selective attention, and attentional shift/flexibility. McDowd and Birren (1990) defined sustained attention/concentration as maintaining mental effort on a task over a period of time; whereas, the authors stated that divided attention refers to the ability to perform two different tasks simultaneously. At a more basic functional level, selective attention is defined as the ability to filter out irrelevant stimuli in the environment and to focus on goal-relevant information. Attentional shiftlflexibility refers to the process by which an individual can shift his/her attention between sets of stimuli, and alternately monitor both sources of information (LaRue, 1992; McDowd & Birren, 1990). Other investigators have described various factors of attention. In an early study which attempted to identify processes involved in attention, Sack and Rice (1974) found that attention consisted of the factors of selectivity, resistance to distraction, and shifting. Results were based on the administration of specific tests, which were designed to measure these three concepts, and on factor analytic techniques. Stankov (1988), in part aiming to identify attentional factors as separate from other cognitive abilities, found three attentional factors: search, concentration, and attentional flexibility. Stankov also stated that support in the literature for the existence of independent selective attention and divided attention factors is lacking. Four factors of attention were posited by Mirsky (1989). Using clinical measures, the factors identified were focus and execute, vigilance, encode, and shift. Similar to Stankov (1988), Shum, McFarland, and Bain (1990), also employing clinical measures of attention, found only three factors of attention which they labeled visual-motor scanning, sustained selective processing, and visual/auditory scanning. In a subsequent study, Shum, McFarland, and Bain (1994) attempted to explore the relationship between the clinical approach of using psychological tests to assess attention and the information processing approach of assessing attention by using experimental tasks based on theories of cognitive abilities. The authors found that the three factors of attention that they had previously derived could be predicted by the following six indices of information processing: mean reaction time, mean movement time, feature extraction, identification, response selection, and motor adjustment. Thus, the authors concluded that both the psychological testing and the information processing approaches are measuring some of the same attentional processes. Although researchers often employ varying operational definitions of attention, it has been theorized that these categories form a common system of attention (Posner & Rafal, 1987). According to van Zomeren and Brouwer (1994), it is unclear how many "distinct focused, divided, and sustained attention abilities exis " (p.29). These authors describe a general, higher level of attention that they refer to as Supervisory Attentional Control. Supervisory Attentional Control in turn influences/controls the two dimensions of attention (selectivity and intensity). The selectivity dimension includes focused attention and divided attention; whereas, the intensity dimension consists of alertness and sustained attention (van Zomeren & Brouwer, 1994). Schmidt, Trueblood, and Mewvin (1994) performed a factor analysis on commonly used clinical tests of attention and identified a clear one-factor solution. This factor may represent a global dimension of attention which perhaps is similar to the Supervisory Attentional Control described by van Zomeren and Brouwer (1994); however, Schmidt et al. (1994) stated that there is little existing information to guide a conceptual understanding of this one factor. As can been seen from the above review of the literature, there are many varying views of the operational definitions and components/factors of attention. Thus, the reporting of mixed findings from studies of attention in older adults in part may be due to employing different specific operational definitions of attention (LaRue, 1992). Theories (flie Wf Attention Reduced Processingfiesources In general, the reduced processing resources theory attributes age-related decrements in cognitive functioning to a reduction in some vital processing resource; however, the exact nature of this processing resource is not often described. Salthouse (1988) concluded that most of the literature regarding processing resources could be described using the metaphors of space, energy, and time. His space metaphor refers to the notion that a finite working-memory capacity exists which controls the amount of short-term storage that is possible simultaneously. The energy metaphor refers to the literature that describes the essential processing resources as attentional capacity. In this view, age-related decrements in attentional abilities are due to a decrease in the energy that drives cognitive processing (Hartley, 1992; Plude & Hoyer, 1985; Salthouse, 1988, 1991). The metaphor of time is based on the notion that the faster cognitive processes can be performed, the more likely multiple operations will be completed accurately (Salthouse, 1988). According to a review of the literature by Salthouse (1988), between 24 and 30% of the age-associated decrements in cognitive functioning, including memory, can be attributed to the mediation of a reduction in the amount of processing resources. However, the specific role of reduced energy as representing a decrease in attentional capacity in older adults is somewhat unclear and is based more on speculation than empirical research (Salthouse, 1988). Currently, the reduced attentional energy hypothesis is not well defined enough to make solid, testable predictions (Hartley, 1992). fltomatic versus_Ejforfiufl Processing Hasher and Zacks (1979) reported that cognitive processes differ in their attentional requirements, which run along a continuum. The authors stated that at either end of the 'attentional requirement' continuum are automatic and effortful processes. Automatic processes "occur. . . without intention, . . . without necessarily giving rise to awareness, and . . . without interfering with other processing" (Hasher & Zacks, 1979, p. 362). These processes cannot be consciously inhibited, and they require very little attentional energy. In contrast, Hasher and Zacks described effortful processes, such as imagery, mnemonic devices, clustering, and rehearsal, as voluntary, controlled processes that require greater amounts of attention and effort. Thus, the number of effortful processes in which an individual can engage simultaneously is limited. Hasher and Zacks (1979) reported that decrements in attentional capacity in old age only affect the efficiency and use of effortful processes. As the occurrence of automatic processes requires only minimal attentional capacity, these processes should remain relatively stable into old age. In support of the idea that the ability to perform tasks requiring effortful processes declines with old age, Craik and McDowd (1987), using cued recall tasks (recall involving the presentation of cues) and recognition memory tasks (involving the re-presentation of material) with young and elderly adults, found that older individuals recalled words significantly more poorly than younger TH A! individuals. Furthermore, older adults performed less well on recall tasks than on recognition tasks. The authors concluded that recall requires more effortful processes than recognition, and thus older adults perform more poorly on tasks requiring recall. Light and Singh (1987) compared the performance of younger and older adults on implicit and explicit memory tasks. Implicit memory involves processes which are relatively automatic and require very little attention; in contrast, explicit memory involved in recall and recognition requires conscious effort and attention. Similar to Craik and McDowd's (1987) findings, results indicated that both young and old adults performed similarly on implicit memory tests. The authors found that older adults' ability to recall words was significantly worse than that of younger adults; however, the study also revealed that older adults performed more poorly on recognition tasks than younger adults, although the authors did not compare recall and recognition abilities in older adults. Further support for the stability of automatic processes in old age was provided by Jennings and Jacoby (1993). Their findings indicated that automatic processes associated with familiarity with names did not differ across young and old adults. Titov and Knight (1997) also found that automatic processes did not differ between younger, middle-aged, and older persons, but that effortful/controlled processes for middle-aged and older adults differed from that of younger participants. Spatial Localization Hflothesis Although the present study does not deal directly with visual search or visual memory, older adults seem to experience some difficulties in these areas. Thus, visual attention abilities of the elderly have been examined in order to assess the impact on other cognitive functioning. Plude and Hoyer (1985) reported that the age-deficits in the visual selective attentional abilities of older adults are a result of a decrease in spatial localization capacities. Spatial localization refers to the "ability to locate task-relevant information in the visual field" (Plude & Hoyer, 1985, p.76). Rather than being viewed as a resource which is reduced with advancing age, spatial localization is conceptualized as a specific process involved in visual information processing (Hartley, 1992). According to Plude and Hoyer (1985), as spatially locating relevant stimuli becomes more difficult and requires more attentional energy as individuals age, less attentional capacity is available for other cognitive processes, such as memory. Support for age-related decrements in spatial localization exists based on studies of visual search (Hartley, 1992; Plude & Hoyer, 1985). Farkas and Hoyer (1980) used a search task to examine the effects of differences between target stimuli and irrelevant information on selective attention abilities. When elderly participants were informed that target items would be in the same position for each trial and contrasting irrelevant information was present, their localization performance was similar to that of younger adults. Similarly, Madden (1983) found that using spatial cueing to inform older adults about the location of a THl target helped to diminish the interference effects of nontarget distracter stimuli. However, other evidence indicates that spatial cues benefit both older and younger adults equally (Nissen & Corkin, 1985). Some further support for the spatial localization hypothesis was provided by Plude and Hoyer (1986). The authors used search (target location is varied) and nonsearch (target location is fixed) tasks, and consistent with earlier findings, reported that nontargets interfered with the detection of targets more significantly for the older participants than younger participants, but only when the task involved search. In addition, target acuity was equated across conditions in order to account for an age-related decrement in parafoveal acuity (Cerella, 1985). Plude and Hoyer (1986) concluded that their results may lend support for the idea that spatial localization abilities decline with advancing age; however, they also stated that an alternative explanation of their findings is the existence of a general age-reduction in the capacity or speed of attentional processes. In a similar vein, Madden and Allen (1991) concluded that a slowing of central processing speed in the elderly could explain performance on a visual search task. Debate still exists in the literature as to the implications of older adults' performance on visual search tasks (McDowd & Birren, 1990); nevertheless, decreased visual selective attentional performance of older adults, which may indirectly affect memory, may be explained, at least in part, by the spatial localization hypothesis. 10 Inhibition The finding that elderly individuals can have difficulty in selectively attending only to relevant stimuli and the results from the selective attention studies discussed above (Farkas & Hoyer, 1980; Madden, 1983; McDowd & Birren, 1990; Plude & Hoyer, 1986) have also been explained as being a result of an age-related decrement in a central processing resource, inhibitory functioning. Hasher and Zacks (1988) proposed that older adults have a reduction in their inhibitory attentional mechanisms. This reduction, in turn, permits irrelevant information to have access to activation and subsequently to enter working memory. In support of this idea, Hasher and Zacks (1988) reported that older adults have a greater number of intrusions of personal memories when attempting to recall text than do younger adults. Older adults seem to be deficient in their inhibition of irrelevant thoughts that may be unintentionally activated by the experimental materials. Results from a study by Kane, Hasher, Stoltzfus, Zacks, and Connelly (1994) lend support for the notion of age-reduction in inhibitory processes. The authors attempted to increase the ability of younger and older adults to suppress attending to distracter stimuli. Findings revealed that although younger participants were able to increase their ability to suppress irrelevant information, older participants did not demonstrate a reliable suppression effect. Kane et al. (1994) concluded that older adults exhibit age-related decrements in inhibitory attentional processes related to identifying distracter items. Similarly, West and Bell (1997) demonstrated that older adults have more difficulty than younger 11 adults do in inhibiting the interference from an incongruent word when attempting to identify the color in which the word was printed. In an effort to investigate age-related auditory selective attention deficits, McDowd and Filion (1992) examined younger and older adults' allocation of attention to relevant and irrelevant stimuli. Participants were instructed to either attend to or ignore certain tones, and skin conductance orienting responses were measured as an index of attentional allocation. Unlike the younger adults, older individuals did not habituate to the 'ignored' stimuli; instead, older participants continued to orient to both relevant and irrelevant stimuli. Similar to Hasher and Zacks (1988) and Kane et al. (1994), McDowd and Filion concluded that older adults have decrements in their ability to inhibit attention to irrelevant information. As mentioned above, one implication of the age-deficit in inhibition is that elderly individuals will experience more difficulty with correctly retrieving items from memory. Gerard, Zacks, Hasher, and Radvansky (1991) found that older adults were more susceptible to the interference effects of irrelevant data when retrieving information from working memory. Gerard et al. (1991) concluded that these results fit with Hasher and Zack's theory that age-decrements in the inhibition of attention allow for more irrelevant information to enter working memory. Thus, retrieval of relevant information from memory is more difficult as it may become conceptually linked with irrelevant data. In contrast, research conducted in the 19605 and 1970s generally did not reveal deficits in inhibitory attentional abilities in older adults (Gerard et al., 1991). No significant differences between younger and older adults in the 12 effects of interference on learning and memory were found. Gerard et al. (1991) suggested that nonsignificant results during these decades might have been due to the emphasis placed on recall measures rather than on the types of errors made and response times. However, findings from a more recent study by Plude and Doussard-Roosevelt (1989) did not support the age-decrement in inhibition hypothesis. The performance of older adults did not differ from that of younger adults, even when the number of distracter items was increased. Thus, although support exists for an age decrease in attentional inhibition, it is not clear whether this is a general problem involved in aging. The possible connection between age-associated deficits in attentional processes and memory processes in older age does merit further examination. In general, a great deal of research indicates that there is an age- associated decrement in attentional abilities which can reduce the efficiency of cognitive processes, including memory; however, interpreting and generalizing the specific nature of attentional deficits can be difficult as these studies employed various operational definitions and tests of attention (Hartley, 1992; Salthouse, 1988). Furthermore, many theoretical explanations of the aging of attention exist and research results can lend support to one or more of these theories; thus, the precise manner in which attentional abilities decrease with age and the processes by which this affects the cognitive and memory abilities of older individuals remains somewhat unclear. 13 Memory in Older Adults It is widely accepted in the literature that aging has a negative impact on memory performance. As memory deficits in healthy older adults are reported frequently, the National Institute of Mental Health proposed the concept of and developed the criteria for "Age-Associated Memory lmpainnent (AAMI)." In addition, the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders - Fourth Edition (DSM-IV; American Psychiatric Association, 1994) has established the diagnostic category of "Age-Related Cognitive Decline." Individuals manifesting AAMI or Age-Related Cognitive Decline are considered to be experiencing normal age-related changes in memory functioning and can be distinguished from persons suffering from memory impairments as a result of dementia or other neurological conditions (Crook, Bartus, Ferris, Whitehouse, Cohen, & Gershon, 1986; DSM-IV, 1994). Although the notion that healthy older adults experience memory difficulties is widely accepted, disagreement exists as to the exact nature of these memory deficits. In addition, it has been reported that atrophy of the hippocampus is related to mild memory impairments in normal older adults (Golomb, de Leon, Kluger, George, Tarshish, & Ferris, 1993). However, clear explanations of why differences exist between younger and older adults' memory abilities have not been provided (LaRue, 1992). It is important to continue to clarify which aspects of memory are most affected by normal aging and which processes are responsible for these changes in order to enhance differential diagnosis of memory disorder, as well as to provide the most effective 14 cognitive retraining programs as possible for older adults (Mitrushina, Satz, Chervinsky, & D'Elia, 1991). Models of Memory Memory §vstems A memory systems account for age-related decrements and variability in memory performance was provided by Tulving (1972, 1983, & 1985). In this view, a distinction is made between the types of information to be remembered. Tulving (1972) introduced the terms "episodic memory" and "semantic memory" to distinguish between two categories of information in memory. He described episodic memory as consisting of information for the events that happen in an individual's life, including information presented in a laboratory/experimental situation. Opposingly, semantic memory was proposed to consist of "the body of organized, general, and highly overlearned information comprising an individual's declarative knowledge system" (Salthouse, 1991, p.241). Later, Tulving (1985) described "procedural memory" (memory from earlier experiences influencing later performance) as a third system which he considered the most evolutionarily and developmentally primitive system. He suggested that the memory systems are organized in a monohierarchical fashion in which procedural memory supports semantic memory that in turn supports episodic memory. Thus, according to this view, episodic memory is the most vulnerable to the effects of aging and trauma; whereas, procedural memory is the least vulnerable (Craik, 1994) 15 It is generally believed that episodic memory is affected by advancing age; whereas, procedural and semantic memory remain stable or are affected only minimally by the aging process (Craik, 1994; Craik & Jennings, 1992). However, Salthouse (1991) disputed this notion somewhat with his conclusion that episodic and semantic memory are not as easily distinguished by the effects of increasing age as generally thought. According to this view, although age differences are larger for tasks involving episodic memory than semantic memory, conclusions regarding the effects of aging on these two types of memory should be tentative at this time. Information-Processing Models Processing models of memory distinguish between types of memory. The process of remembering begins with information being stored for a second or less in sensory store/memory. Then information is transferred to primary/short- tenn memory where it is held for approximately 30 seconds; this kind of memory has also been referred to as immediate memory and is considered to be capacity-limited in the amount of information that can be held at any one moment (Kausler, 1994). After being rehearsed in primary memory, the information subsequently is transferred to secondary/Iong-term memory, which involves the preservation of information in memory from a few minutes to years and is considered to be capacity-unlimited (Salthouse, 1991). The final stage of memory involves the transferring of information to tertiary/remote memory, where information is maintained for several years to decades. 16 The differential effects of aging on each stage of memory processing in the information-processing model have been examined. In general, when speed of processing is considered as an indicator of functioning, all of the stages of processing appear affected by age (LaRue, 1992). However, when response accuracy is employed as a measure of functioning, evidence for the effects of age on sensory, primary, and tertiary memory is less confirming. Evidence remains more convincing for the detrimental effects of aging on secondary memory (LaRue, 1992; Salthouse, 1991). D_egth-of-ProcessinLModel According to Craik and Lockhart (1972), the dichotomy between short- terrn memory stores and long-term memory stores is somewhat artificial. They proposed that memory is based on levels of perceptual processing which run along a continuum of analysis. Early stages of processing involve analysis of sensory features; whereas, later stages are responsible for "pattern recognition and the extraction of meaning" (Craik & Lockhart, 1972, p.675). Thus, the greater the extent of cognitive analysis, the greater the depth of processing. As a result of this perceptual processing, memory traces are formed. The authors suggested that the durability/strength of the trace depends on the depth of analysis (i.e., the deeper the level of processing, the stronger and more complex the memory trace). A disadvantage of the depth-of-processing model is that it tends to lead to general principles of operation rather than to specific ° mechanisms of memory which can be empirically tested (Craik & Jennings, 1992) 17 As discussed previously, Craik and McDowd (1987) found that older adults performed more poorly on measures of recall than on those of recognition. These results can be viewed in terms of Craik and Lockhart's (1972) depth-of- processing theory. Older adults may be deficient in their ability to process information at a deep level; thus, recall tasks (which involve retrieving information from memory with few to no cues) are more difficult for them than recognition tasks (which involve the re-presentation of information for recognition). Along these lines, Craik and McDowd described their findings of older adult's memory deficits (particularly on tasks of recall) in terms of these individuals being adversely affected by a reduction in processing resources (Craik, 1994; Craik & McDowd, 1987). This is similar to Hasher and Zacks' (1979) theory that older adults experience deficits in effortful processing. Findings that older individuals use clustering/chunking strategies less frequently than younger individuals when presented with learning tasks support the view that the aging process decreases effortful and deeper processing abilities (Eysenck, 1974; Mungas, Ehlers, & Blunden, 1991). W Much research has been devoted to investigating specific cognitive changes that are responsible for the age-related decrements in memory performance. As memory is also commonly viewed as involving the processes of encoding, storage, and retrieval, efforts have been undertaken to discover whether deficits in these processes affect secondary memory deficits in older adults. Alternate terminology for these 18 processes includes registration/acquisition, retention, and recall, respectively. These processes do influence each other (e.g., if one fails to encode information, then it likewise will not be stored nor can it be retrieved); however, these processes are usually investigated separately. As researchers have had difficulty defining storage in cognitive terms, studies have generally been more focused on the effects of age on the processes of encoding and retrieval (Craik & Jennings, 1992). Research regarding age differences in encoding has yielded mixed results. In several studies, encoding ability has been measured by assessing older adults' performance on tasks of recognition of words. As recognition tasks provide external cues, which significantly decrease processing demands, the effects of storage and retrieval on performance are minimized (Mitrushina & Satz, 1989). Mitrushina and Satz (1989) and Mitrushina et al. (1991) found that four age groups of older adults did not differ in their performance on recognition tasks of learned material. The authors concluded that older adults' ability to encode information is unaffected by the aging process. However, this study did not compare older participants to younger participants. Similarly, as discussed previously, Craik and McDowd (1987) found that older adults' performance on a task of recognition fell at a somewhat higher level than the performance of younger adults. The mean age of the older participants was 72.8 years; whereas, the mean age of the younger participants was 20.7 years. Thus, this study also seems to indicate that the encoding abilities of older adults remain intact. In contrast to these findings, Light and Singh (1987) found sizable age 19 differences between older and younger adults on a recognition task. However, participants in this study were not initially instructed to remember target words. Overall, the findings reported in the literature regarding age differences in encoding are relatively discrepant, and the numerous methods employed by these studies make interpretation of the discrepancies difficult. Various individual factors may moderate memory performance and need further consideration, as some research findings have indicated that education, intellectual activity, and level of income can predict recall abilities in older adults (Arbuckle, Gold, & Andres, 1986; Craik, Byrd, and Swanson, 1987). Retrieval processes more generally have been found to be negatively affected by increasing age (Craik & Jennings, 1992; Mitrushina & Satz, (1989); Mitrushina et al. (1991), & Morris, Craik, & Gick, 1990). Retrieval abilities of older adults have frequently been evaluated using measures of free recall (recall unaided by cues or re-presentation of material) of words/word lists. As recall tasks require more effort and must be "self-initiated" rather than guided by external information, it follows that recall should be more adversely affected by the aging process than recognition (Craik & Jennings, 1992, p.82). Craik and McDowd (1987), as discussed previously, found that- older adults performed more poorly on recall than recognition, thus suggesting that recall requires more effortful retrieval processes than does recognition. However, Salthouse (1991) suggested that findings of older adults having greater recognition versus recall abilities may be a result of the great variation in the number or type of I— ‘- l ‘1. l. -_.l:l alternatives provided in recognition tasks in different studies. No standard test of recognition exists. In summary, findings regarding the effects of aging on memory abilities indicate that episodic memory is more adversely affected by aging than semantic and procedural memory. Likewise, secondary memory abilities have been more conclusively found to decrease with increasing age than sensory, primary, or tertiary memory abilities. Furthermore, older adults may have increasing difficulty with effortful processing of information at an adequately deep level. This may in turn contribute to the more general finding that older individuals perform more poorly than younger individuals on tasks of retrieval involving free recall. Findings regarding the effects of aging on encoding, as represented by performance on tasks of recognition, have been more discrepant. Verbal Memom As is apparent from the above discussion, age-related deficits in memory tend to vary according to the aspects of memory being assessed and methodology being employed. The studies described in the preceding paragraphs measured verbal memory as represented by memory for words/word lists, as is commonly found in the literature on memory and aging. This is likely a result, at least in part, of elderly adults fre’quently complaining of verbal memory difficulties, such as not remembering an individual's name after an introduction or not remembering what they or others have just said (Kausler, 1985). In addition, a recent study found that verbal memory processes activate additional brain regions than those usually associated with memory (medic-temporal and 21 diencephalic structures). Findings implicated the left prefrontal cortex and retrosplenial area in the encoding of verbal material and the right prefrontal cortex and the precuneus in the retrieval of this material (Shallice, Fletcher, Frith, Grasby, Frackowlak, & Dolan, 1994), although support for these findings is limited and conclusions are speculative. Schaie and Willis (1993) collected cross-sectional data for the 5th wave of the Seattle Longitudinal Study in an attempt to discover patterns of age differences across different ability domains, including verbal memory. In this study, verbal memory was defined as "the ability to encode, store, and recall meaningful language units," and was represented by measures of immediate recall, delayed recall, and word fluency. The authors found that older adults performed more poorly on word and list learning than younger adults, but that younger adults had more difficulty on the word fluency task than older adults. The authors stated that this may be a result of list Ieaming involving episodic memory and verbal fluency involving semantic memory. Schaie and Willis also found that women performed significantly better than men on all of the verbal memory measures; verbal IQ was not controlled, although no differences in gender were found on a measure of verbal ability. This perhaps indicates that women, during their lifetime, develop better verbal skills than men which thus may result in greater verbal memory abilities in old age. Using derived ability factor scores, findings also indicated that younger adults have greater verbal memory ability than older adults. 22 A study by Hultsch, Hertzog, and Dixon (1990) investigated the ability correlates of verbal memory as related to adulthood and aging. The sample consisted of 584 males and females in three age groups (19-36, 55-69, and 70- 86 years). Verbal memory was measured by recall of words and of text. Similar to Schaie and Willis' study (1993), results indicated that younger adults outperformed older adults and men performed more poorly than women on verbal memory tasks. Additional results indicated that the variance associated with age was greatly reduced when differences in individual cognitive processing variables were taken into account. These individual differences in ability, which included semantic speed, comprehension speed, working memory, verbal fluency, clustering, and reading comprehension, were found to account for approximately half of the variance in text and word recall. A related study by Hultsch, Hertzog, Small, McDonald-Miszczak, and Dixon (1992) examined whether evidence existed for longitudinal change over the course of three years in cognitive abilities, including verbal working memory, text recall, and word recall. Findings indicated that earlier-born individuals had a larger decrement in verbal working memory performance than the later-bom individuals. The authors did not find a decline in performance on the text and word recall measures; however, they suggested that a longer time interval may be required to observe these predicted decrements. As mentioned above, clustering or semantic organization (grouping related information into categories) of verbal material may enhance verbal memory abilities (Burke & Light, 1981; Hultsch et al., 1990; Kausler, 1994). 23 cl E Di lh Frequently, studies investigating the categorical organizational abilities of older adults compare memory performance on free-recall trials to that on cued-recall trials. According to Burke and Light (1981), semantic organization abilities in older adults are not as stable as had been previously thought; however, conflicting findings exist in the literature as a result of the varying paradigms used. It has been suggested that more passive forms of semantic processing are unaffected by the aging process; whereas, more active forms of categorical clustering (independently discovering semantic relationships) are subject to age- related decrements (Dobbs, Aubrey, & Rule, 1989). This may be a result of older adults experiencing a decrease in their effortful processing abilities. Along these lines, elderly individuals' verbal memory abilities may be enhanced when logical contextual connections are provided, such as with a short story (Meyer & Rice, cited in Albert, 1984). In general, results of studies examining the verbal memory abilities of older adults indicate that these individuals tend to perform worse than younger individuals on verbal learning and memory tasks. The effects of gender may also need to be considered. Furthermore, older adults tend to have deficits in active semantic organization abilities which may be a result of decreased effortful processing abilities. The Relationship between Dew As evidence from the above discussion indicated, memory abilities of older adults may be enhanced through various semantic techniques; however, the memory functioning of older adults may also be hindered by depression. 24 Among older adults, depression is the second most common mental disorder; only dementia occurs more frequently (LaRue, 1992). Depression has been found to occur for the first time in 10-20% of adults 60 years of age and older (Ruegg, Zisook, & Swerdlow, 1988). Depression has been investigated as both a clinical syndrome, in which symptoms must meet various criteria in order to receive a diagnosis, and as a range of symptoms which vary in severity and are assessed by using self-report scales (Karel, 1997). Although older adults have been found to have the lowest rates of the clinical syndrome, major depression, adults aged approximately 70 years and older tend to report the greatest number of depressive symptoms when compared to adults in their late 305 to late 603, although further and more general conclusions cannot be drawn as a result of the diverse measurements used in this type of research (Karel, 1987; Kessler, Foster, Webster, & House, 1992; Newmann, 1989). As depression is considered to be multidetermined, Karel (1997) investigated the factors that may influence the development of depression in older adults. The author found that as an individual ages, helshe experiences an increase in biological vulnerability to depression as a result of an increase in the "prevalence of multiple medical and neurological disorders," as well as a result of possible side effects from medication (Karel, 1997, p.858). Furthermore, a gradual increase was found in the number of stressful life events reported by older adults beginning in their late 50s and included chronic illness, disability, and deaths of spouses, family members, and friends; this continual increase in stressful life events may 25 account, at least in part, for the increased reporting of depressive symptoms by very old adults. According to the results of a meta-analytic study, depression and memory are significantly related to one another (Burt, Zembar, & Niederehe, 1995). Depressed persons have been shown to have difficulty using semantic encoding strategies to organize to-be-remembered information (Weingartner, Cohen, Murphy, Martello, & Gerdt, 1981). Furthermore, depressed older adults may exhibit greater levels of cognitive impairment as compared to depressed younger adults. In mild to moderate depression, cognitive deficits experienced by older individuals are usually mild in nature. In contrast, one in five older individuals suffering from more severe depression experience serious cognitive impairments (LaRue, 1992). The most noticeable difference in neuropsychological test performance between severely depressed and nondepressed older adults are on tests of Ieaming and recall and visuospatial processing. There are two views regarding the origins of cognitive deficits which result from depression (Jorm, 1986). The first states that depressed people experience secondary effects of depression. They have the ability to perform well on cognitive tasks, but are unable to do so as a result of reduced motivation, having depressive thoughts and worries, and/or suffering from feelings of learned helplessness. The second theory proposes that the cognitive deficits are intrinsic to and directly caused by the depression itself and not just a secondary cause of depression. Although there is some evidence to support both of these views, little research has been conducted to test each view directly (Jorm, 1986). 26 In re‘ di< Hc Ell de dl‘ di: In any case, evidence does suggest that depressed individuals have difficulty with tasks which require effortful processing, such as tasks of secondary memory and elaborate encoding abilities (Roy-Byme, Weingartner, Bierer, Thompson, & Post, 1986; Weingartner et al., 1981 ). Studies assessing immediate recall of paragraph length stories revealed that depressed older adults performed at a lower level than expected for their age (LaRue, 1992). Their performance was similar to the performance of individuals with mild Alzheimer's disease. In contrast, studies involving list- learning have yielded somewhat more confusing results. LaRue (1992) concluded that depressed patients, as generally diagnosed using DSM-lll-R criteria, have normal learning curves on list-learning tasks; although, results of a study by Sweeney, Wetzler, Stokes, and Kocsis (1989) indicated that severity of depression, as classified by Research Diagnostic Criteria (RDC), is negatively related to performance on initial trials of verbal list-Ieaming tasks. These authors did not find that depression is related to performance on subsequent trials. However, Deptula, Singh, and Pomara (1993), using self-report rating scales to assess levels of depression, found a significant negative relationship between elderly adults' memory performance on a verbal list-Ieaming task and depression. Comparing the findings of these studies is difficult though, as d'rfferent methods were used to assess depression. Although some findings are discrepant, it can be concluded that depression, either as a clinical syndrome or as based on self-report, can cause cognitive and memory deficits, and thus its effects on performance must be controlled in experimental studies. 27 Improvement in Memory after Cognitive Retraining in Older Adults Because older adults experience deficits in memory abilities and frequently complain about these perceived difficulties, efforts have been made to discover effective training programs to improve the verbal memory skills of these individuals. A significant focus in the literature has been on improving elderly persons' recall of word lists and names. The two techniques most commonly used in this endeavor are the method of loci (used for remembering word lists) and the face-name mnemonic (used for remembering names). In addition, some attempts have been made to use attention/concentration training techniques to enhance the memory abilities of older individuals. ILLe—Mgthodf of Loci Improving the abilities of elderly adults to remember lists of words might only seem relevant to laboratory settings; however, there are comparable tasks in daily living which involve this skill (Yesavage & Rose, 1983). For example, remembering needed grocery items, appointments, and/or activities or errands which need to be done all involve list-learning. The method of loci has frequently been used with all age groups in an effort to improve this skill. The method of loci is a visual imagery technique which involves an individual generating and Ieaming in order a list of locations from a familiar surrounding. Then helshe creates a visual image which involves the first list item to-be- remembered and the first location from his/her loci list. For example, if the first item is a loaf of bread and needs to be remembered with the location of a circle drive, the subject would picture a large puffy circular loaf of bread floating around the circular drive. 28 Each subsequent item on the to-be-remembered list is then matched with each subsequent location. In order to recall the list of words, the individual must take a mental walk through the pathway of their loci and remember the visual image and item associated with each location (Yesavage & Rose, 1984). Anschutz, Camp, Markley, and Kramer (1985) trained 10 older adults to use the method of loci in order to remember a list of nouns and a list of grocery items. Training sessions consisted of two 60 minute sessions. Participants were tested on their recall of nouns prior to and eight weeks after training. Results indicated that recall significantly improved. For the grocery list, each subject generated a list of 30 words, 12 of which were chosen for recall. Participants were taken grocery shopping and required to shop for the 12 items. Four weeks later, this task was repeated with different grocery items. Near perfect performance was found for both grocery trials. Results also revealed that participants who continued to use the method of loci technique during the second grocery trial performed better during the final free recall task than those who modified their strategies. However, caution needs to be taken when interpreting these results, as only 10 participants were used and no control group existed. A study by Kliegl, Smith, and Baltes (1989) used the method of loci technique to examine the cognitive plasticity of the abilities of elderly adults. The serial recall of words was tested for a group of 20 older adults (mean age = 71.7 years) and compared to that of a group of 4 younger adults (mean age = 22.8 years). Pretesting was conducted prior to the administration of 26 training 29 sessions to both groups; posttesting was then completed. Results indicated that both the older and younger groups improved significantly in their serial recall. However, younger adults' performance improved to a greater extent than the performance of older adults. The authors concluded that younger adults have greater cognitive plasticity than older adults, but that older adults do have an adequate level of plasticity in order to benefit from method of loci training. It is important to note however that the number of training sessions each subject received depended upon their performance; thus, participants did not receive uniform training. In addition, once again, the generalizability of these results may be questioned in that the younger subject group only consisted of four individuals and results were not compared to a control group. Kliegl et al. (1989) were able to replicate the above results using groups of 18 young adults and 19 older adults, thus increasing the ability to generalize their findings. Baltes and Kliegl (1992) extended Kliegl et al.'s (1989) work by increasing the number of training sessions to a standard of 38. As with the Kliegl et al. (1989) study, these authors found that both older and younger adults' recall performance improved after method of loci training, but that younger adults' performance improved significantly more than older adults'. Thus, these results strengthen Kliegl et al.'s (1989) conclusion that although older adults have an adequate level of cognitive plasticity for retraining, younger adults‘ have a significantly greater amount of cognitive plasticity, though again no control group was used. 30 Yesavage & Rose (1984b) investigated the effectiveness of two variations of the method of loci technique. They trained one group of older adults to perform the standard method of loci technique and another group of older adults to perform the method of loci technique and to make judgments of the pleasantness of each visual image association. In contrast to the previously discussed results, findings indicated that the number of items recalled improved only for the older adults in the Loci + Judgment group. Yesavage and Rose (1984b) did find that both groups in their study improved in the number of items that were recalled in the correct order. The authors concluded that these results may be due to differences in methodology between studies. This study trained participants during three sessions; whereas, the Kliegl et al. (1989) and Baltes and Kliegl (1992) studies used approximately 26 and 38 sessions, respectively. Once again, however, no control group was used. Thus, differences in methodologies and the failure to include control groups may hinder the generalizability of results; although, in general, studies examining the effectiveness of the method of loci technique provide evidence that this type of training may improve the recall memory abilities of older adults. The Fag-flame Mnemonic Due to the difficulty older adults experience in recalling names, imagery techniques, such as the face-name mnemonic, have been developed in an effort to improve this type of recall (Yesavage, Rose, & Bower, 1983). The face-name mnemonic technique developed by McCarty (1980) involves a) identifying a prominent feature of the face; b) modifying the person's name into a high- 31 imagery object ; and c) associating the facial feature with the high-imagery object through the use of visual imagery. In order to recall a person's name, one must recall the prominent feature of the face, which should subsequently aid in the recall of the image association. The transformed name must then be recalled and decoded into the original name (Yesavage et al. 1983). For example, in order to remember that a woman's name is Pearl, one could identify that she has a prominent smile, then could visualize her mouth opening wide to reveal pearls dancing around inside. When seeing her later, one would think of her mouth, the pearls dancing around inside of it, and her name, "Pearl." Yesavage and Rose (1984a) trained 67 participants, divided into young, middle-aged, and older adult groups, with the face-name mnemonic technique. Training sessions were provided on two consecutive days. Recall of names was assessed prior to and after training. Results indicated that all three groups significantly improved in their recall of names and that level of improvement was equivalent for the groups. Although a control group was not included, the authors stated that these findings suggest that the face-name mnemonic technique is equally effective in improving the memory abilities of young, middle- aged, and older adults. Similar to the previously described Yesavage and Rose (1984b) investigation of the combination of imagery and affective judgment, Yesavage et al. (1983) examined the effects of training elderly adults to use the face-name mnemonic combined with making judgments about the pleasantness of their visual image association. The recall performance of this groups was compared 32 to the performance of a group trained only in the face-name mnemonic technique and to a control group which was not taught an imagery technique. Training consisted of one session per day for five consecutive days. Unlike the Yesavage and Rose (1984b) findings, the Yesavage et al. (1983) results indicated that although the face-name mnemonic and judgment group performed better than the face-name mnemonic only group, the recall of both of these groups improved significantly after training when compared to the control group. As previously mentioned, Yesavage and Rose (1984b) study had only three training sessions; whereas, the Yesavage et al. (1983) study had five sessions. The increase in the number of training sessions may have contributed to these more positive findings; however, the results of the two studies are somewhat difficult to compare as each used a different imagery technique. In general, Yesavage et al. (1983) stated that their findings increase confidence in the use of face-name mnemonics as a memory training technique. Further support for the effectiveness of the face-name mnemonic technique was found by Gratzinger, Sheikh, Friedman, and Yesavage (1990). These authors examined the name recall differences between groups of older adults who received farm- name mnemonic training combined with either additional imagery training, relaxation training, or both additional imagery and affective judgment training. No differences in recall were found between groups; however, the participants' recall performance in all three groups significantly improved after training. Yesavage (1984) investigated relaxation training on the effectiveness of the face-name mnemonic training on recall performance. Thirty-nine elderly 33 participants were divided into two groups and received six training sessions. The first group received relaxation training followed by face-name mnemonic training, and the second group received nonspecific techniques on improving one's attitude followed by face-name mnemonic training. Findings indicated that participants in both groups improved in their recall ability after training, but that the individuals in the combination mnemonic and relaxation training group improved to a greater extent than those in the mnemonic and nonspecific technique training group. Yesavage (1984) concluded that relaxation may help to reduce anxiety levels in order to be able to more effectively use mnemonic strategies. These results also provide further support for the effectiveness of the face-name mnemonic training; although, again a control group receiving no training was not used. Similar to Yesavage (1984), Yesavage and Jacob (1984) combined relaxation and the face-name mnemonic training in an effort to improve name recall for elderly adults. However, only one group of participants was used in this study, and two training sessions were conducted. Participants' recall was tested three times: prior to relaxation training, after relaxation training, and after the face-name mnemonic training. Results indicated that recall improved after both the relaxation and mnemonic training had been given, but not after the relaxation training alone. Yesavage and Jacob (1984) also examined the effects of this combined training on the relationship between name-face recall and the attentional abilities of these participants. Using a computerized divided attention task to evaluate attentional abilities, the authors found that relaxation training 34 improved the participants' attention, and that better attention performance significantly correlated with improvements in name-face recall. However, the task used to evaluate attention in this study is specific to one component of attention. In addition, the attention/name-face recall correlation does not imply causation and is difficult to interpret. Generally though, it does seem that the name-face mnemonic training technique is effective in improving name-face recall abilities of older adults, and that additional training in relaxation and in making affective judgments may enhance these effects. Atte_ntionlConcengation Training Studies examining the effects of attention/concentration training on the cognitive abilities of older adults are sparse. Furthermore, most of the existing studies have examined the training effects on elderly adults' attentional abilities, rather than their memory functioning. Willis, Cornelius, Blow, and Baltes (1983) did examine the impact of attention training on older adults' memory span abilities, although the major focus of their study was aimed at improving the attentional abilities of these participants. Seventy-three elderly adults received five attention training sessions which were intended to enhance four dimensions of attention: discrimination, selective attention, attention switching, and concentration- vigilance. Results indicated that attention training did improve older adults' attentional abilities; however, this training did not have an effect on memory span abilities. It should be noted that Willis et al. (1983) only used attention training 35 sessions in this study and did not incorporate any type of memory training into their protocol. Additionally, no control group was used. Yesavage and Rose (1983) investigated the effectiveness of the combination of concentration and memory training on older adults' memory abilities. Two groups were used for this study. One group received concentration training (which included relaxation training) followed by the method of loci training, and the other group received the two types of training in the reverse order. Concentration training involved techniques to enhance selective and sustained attention, such as noting and recalling details from pictures and performing list-learning tasks with background verbal interference. Findings indicated that although both groups' ability for immediate and delayed recall pairs of words and text improved after training, the group receiving concentration training prior to the method of loci training performed significantly better than the group that received training in the reverse order. The authors concluded that elderly persons who receive concentration training before memory training may derive greater benefits from this latter type of training. It is important to note, however, that relaxation training was included as a component of concentration training in this study, and thus it is difficult to disentangle which training factors may have influenced results. Furthermore, no control group was included in this study. Rockstroh, Dietrich, and Pokorny (1995) used a somewhat different method of attention and memory training in an attempt to improve attention and memory abilities. Twenty-four elderly and 23 younger participants took part in 36 two training periods, each of which consisted of four sessions, which involved practice training using parallel test versions to baseline and outcome measures. The tests used measured orientation, focused attention, verbal memory, and nonverbal memory. The authors found that the performance of both elderly and young participants improved on all measures after practice training. Rockstroh et al. (1995) concluded that older adults' cognitive test performance can be improved by training. However, it is questionable whether the improvements found by having elderly adults practice skills by taking parallel test versions of post-tests can be generalized to other tests and real-world tasks which involve attention and memory abilities. Similar to Yesavage and Rose (1983), Neely and Backman (1995) also examined the effects of attention training combined with the method of loci and relaxation training. Training was administered in the same order to a group of 23 older adults. An additional 23 older adults did not receive any training and thus served as a control group. Attention training involved focused and divided attention tasks, as well as working-memory tasks. Results revealed that the elderly adults who received the combined training immediately improved in their recall of concrete words and were able to maintain this improvement after six months. In contrast, control participants' recall did not improve. Additionally, neither age, years of education, nor Mini-Mental State Examination scores were predictors of performance gains. The authors suggested that this may be due to the small range of variation in these variables for these participants. Overall, Neely and Backman (1995) concluded that multifactorial training improves older 37 adults' recall; however, as the experimental group received attention, method of loci, and relaxation training, the authors noted that it is unclear which of these training components contributed to their findings. In summary, imagery training techniques involving the method of loci and the face-name mnemonic appear to benefit the memory abilities of older adults. Research also indicates that additional techniques, such as affective judgment training, relaxation training, and attention/concentration training may enhance the benefits of the more traditional mnemonic training. It seems likely that techniques specifically designed to enhance cognitive abilities, such as attention training, may provide more direct benefits to individuals experiencing memory problems than techniques, such as relaxation, which are more broad in focus. However, as discussed above, drawing more specific conclusions regarding the enhancement of memory using attention training techniques is difficult as a result of the combination of attention and relaxation training in these studies. Also, generalizing results to all elderly adults may be questioned as most of these studies did not include groups of untrained participants to serve as controls. It is important to use control groups in these types of treatment studies in order to investigate whether participants can be affected by nonspecific factors. Simply having been included in an experimental group may affect participants' post- treatment performance; this needs to be examined before more definitive conclusions can be drawn regarding the effects of memory, attention, and relaxation training. 38 General Summary and Hypotheses Overall, it can be concluded that attentional abilities decline with increasing age. Resolving conflicting findings is difficult as many studies operationalize attention in a variety of ways. Several theories exist to explain the processes involved in and causes for age-related decrements in attention; however, results in the literature can often be attributed to more than one theory. Elderly individuals also have been found to experience decreases in memory functioning with advancing age. In particular, secondary memory abilities appear most affected by the aging process. Older adults tend to have greater difficulty with tasks of free recall than younger adults. This suggests problems with encoding and/or retrieval; however, studies examining encoding abilities have revealed conflicting findings. In addition, research has shown that active semantic organization abilities of older adults may be deficient. These findings from studies of memory seem compatible with attentional theories involving age- related decreases in effortful processing (Hasher & Zacks, 1979), as well as with memory theories suggesting that depth of processing abilities decrease with increasing age (Craik & Lockhart, 1972). Attentional deficits in elderly adults may contribute to the decreases found in memory functioning. Furthermore, the effects of depression need to be controlled statistically as more severe levels of depression can hinder performance on measures of memory. As attention and memory abilities tend to decline in normal aging, it is important to develop ways in which older adults can improve their memory functioning. Research has shown that memory training techniques involving 39 imagery (the method of loci and the face-name mnemonic) can enhance elderly adults' performance on memory measures. However, the effects of adding additional components, such as attention training, to memory training on older adults' memory abilities are somewhat unclear. It seems plausible though that adding a specific and targeted cognitive training component, such as attention training, to memory training will improve memory functioning more than by adding a more general component, such as relaxation, to memory training; although this needs investigation. In addition, generalizing findings from studies involving memory training is somewhat difficult as control groups frequently have not been used. As research has indicated that older adults experience decreases in their verbal memory abilities and these individuals frequently complain about these difficulties, the present study examined the effects of combining memory and attention training on the verbal memory abilities of healthy older adults. The performance of this group was compared to the performances of a group receiving memory and relaxation training and a control group not receiving any training. Thus, this study expanded and improved upon the methods used in previous studies. Hypotheses These hypotheses are formulated in terms of nine components of the California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT) and the Logical Memory from the Wechsler Memory Scale. Detailed information about these dependent variables is given below under "Measures." Based on the above review of the literature, the following hypotheses were tested: 40 1. Participants who receive combined memory and attention training will perform better on overall verbal memory measures than participants who receive combined memory and relaxation training. Both groups of participants who receive training will perform better on overall verbal memory measures after training than participants in a control group who do not receive training. Specifically, the group that receives memory and attention training will have greater total recall scores on the CVLT and greater total recall scores on LM than the group that receive memory and relaxation training. Both groups will have greater total recall scores on the CVLT and greater total recall scores on LM after training than a control group. 2. Participants who receive combined memory and attention training will demonstrate more active and organized recall strategies than those who receive combined memory and relaxation training. Participants in both groups will demonstrate more active and organized recall strategies after training than those in a control group who do not receive training. Specifically, participants who receive memory and attention training will use semantic clustering strategies more frequently on the CVLT than participants who receive memory and relaxation training. Participants in both training groups will use semantic clustering strategies more frequently on the CVLT after training than participants in a control group. 3. Participants in all groups will perform better on a task of recognition memory than on a task of free recall. However, the performance of those who receive combined memory and attention training on recognition and recall tasks will be 41 better than the performance of those who receive combined memory and relaxation training. The performances of participants in both training groups on recognition and recall tasks will be better after training than the performance of those in a control group. Specifically, participants in all groups will have higher pretraining recognition scores than long-delay free recall scores on the CVLT. Those who receive memory and attention training will have higher recognition and long-delay free recall scores than those who receive memory and relaxation training. Participants in both training groups will have higher recognition and long-delay free recall scores on the CVLT after training than participants in a control group. 4. As older adults tend to benefit from information being presented with cues, all groups of participants will perform better on pretraining cued recall than on unstructured word recall. However, participants in the memory and attention group will improve more than the participants in the memory and relaxation group in their unstructured word recall. Both groups will have higher unstructured word recall scores after training than a control group Specifically, all groups of participants will have higher pretraining short-delay cued recall scores than short-delay free recall scores on the CVLT and higher pretraining long—delay cued recall scores than long-delay free recall scores on the CVLT. However, after training, participants in the memory and attention group will have higher short-delay free recall scores and long-delay free recall scores on the CVLT than those in the memory and relaxation group. Both groups will have higher free recall scores after training than the control group. 42 5. Participants who receive combined memory and attention training will perform better on an unstructured immediate word recall measure than participants who receive combined memory and relaxation training. Both groups of participants who receive training will perform better on unstructured immediate word recall after training than participants in a control group who do not receive training. Specifically, participants who received memory and attention training will have higher CVLT Trial 1 immediate free recall scores than participants who received memory and relaxation training. Both groups will perform better after training than the control group. 43 METHOD Participants The participants for the current study consisted of 84 individuals from an ongoing Michigan State University (MSU) Psychological Clinic research project on aging. The participants were community-dwelling older adult volunteers over the age of 60 who were recruited from senior citizen groups, faculty and staff retiree groups from MSU, and other Mid-Michigan area locations. Advertisements and flyers were used to recruit these individuals. Older adults who earned a score of 24 or greater on the Mini Mental State Examination and did not report any significant history of severe neurological or medical problems (e.g., stroke, significant traumatic brain injury) were included in analyses. In addition, elderly individuals who were experiencing severe levels of depressive symptomatology were not included in the larger study; instead, they were referred for treatment. For the larger study, participants who met the selection criteria (see above) were randomly assigned to three treatment conditions: memory and attention training, memory and relaxation training, and a control condition in which no training was provided. For each of these three groups, the treatment conditions were imposed between a pre-treatment (Time 1) and a post-treatment (Time 2) set of measurements on nine dependent variables. These dependent variables were the eight subscales of the CVLT and LM of the Wechsler Memory Scale. These scales are described below. 44 As part of the larger study, participants were offered two mood and memory assessments and seven memory training sessions. As studies in the literature using between two and eight sessions have revealed similar significant results to studies employing 26-38 sessions, seven sessions for the current study was selected as useful and practical. As described above, individuals were randomly assigned to one of three groups receiving either memory/attention training, memory/relaxation training, or no training. Those not receiving training (control group) were offered to participate in memory training sessions after they had completed their second assessment. The combined memory and attention training group consisted of 40 older adults, the combined memory and relaxation group consisted of 32 older adults, and the control group consisted of 12 older adults. The smaller control group likely is a result of individuals (who were being recruited as control participants) not wanting to participate in the current study when it entailed their completing two full assessments prior to gaining admission to the memory training workshops. According to Cohen (1992), in order to detect an effect size (1) of .40 (corresponding to power = .80) for main effects for between-groups when using ANOVAs for statistical analysis, a sample size of 63 participants (21 participants for each of three groups) is required. As the current study included 84 participants (32 and 40 participants for the memory/attention training and memory/relaxation training groups, respectively, and 12 participants for the control group), this should have been adequate for power to approximate the .80 level. 45 Measures Beck Depression Inventory (BDI) The BDI (Beck, Ward, Mendelson, Mock, & Erbaugh, 1961; see Appendix A) is a self-report instrument which assesses the intensity of depressive symptoms on a 4-point rating scale. Beck (1987) recommended ranges of scores to indicate three levels of depressive symptomatology. According to Beck, scores ranging from 0 - 9 reflect normal functioning, scores ranging from 10 - 19 reflect minimal to mild levels of depressive symptomatology, and scores equal to or greater than 20 reflect moderate to severe levels of depressive symptomatology. Meta-analytic investigation of the BDI revealed that its internal consistency ranged from .73 to .92 (mean = .86) and its concurrent validity as represented by correlation with clinical ratings of psychiatric patients ranged from .55 to .96 (mean =.72; Beck, Steer, & Garbin, 1988). Research also has shown that the BDI has good internal consistency and stability in testing older adults (Beck, Steer, & Garbin, 1988). Using customary cutoff scores, the BDI was able to accurately detect major and minor depressive disorders in elderly, with a misclassification rate of only 16-17% (Gallagher, Nies, 8 Thompson, 1983). Therefore, the BDI can effectively be used to screen for depression in older adufls. Geriatric Depression Scatle (GDS) The GDS (Yesavage, Brink, Rose, Lum, Huang, Adey, & Leirer, 1983; see Appendix B) is a self-report rating scale which was specifically designed to 46 evaluate levels of depressive symptomatology in the elderly. This measure focuses on mood aspects of depression, and minimizes the number of questions regarding somatic symptoms. It consists of 30 yes/no questions which pertain to the feelings of the subject during the past week including the day of testing. The recommended ranges to be used to assess levels of depression are 0 - 9 (normal), 10 - 19 (mild depression), and 20 and above (severe depression). Results of research indicated that the concurrent validity of the GDS, when correlated with the BDI, was .73 (Beck, 1978). Factor analysis of the GDS revealed a major factor, labeled dysphoria, and two minor factors, referred to as worry/obsessive thought and apathy/withdrawal (Pannalee, Katz, & Lawton, 1989). In addition, as measured against the Research Diagnostic Criteria, criterion validity was found to be .82 (Yesavage et al., 1983). Mini-Mental State ExaanaQn (MMSE) The MMSE (Folstein, Folstein, & McHugh, 1975; see Appendix C) was designed to briefly assess cognitive functioning. This instrument assesses the following functions: orientation, immediate recall, attention and calculation, intermediate recall, and language. The maximum obtainable score is 30 points; a score below 24 suggests impairment in cognitive functioning. Scores obtained by the standardization sample for the MMSE, which consisted of 63 healthy older adults, and by younger psychiatric patients ranged from 24.6 to 27.6. The scores for demented individuals ranged from 9.6 to 12.2. In addition, test-retest reliabilities, during a 24-hour period, were reported to be .85 to .99 (Mitrushina & Satz, 1991). 47 Qalifomia Verfl Leami‘npg Test (CVLT) The CVLT (Delis, Kramer, Kaplan, & Ober,1987; see Appendix D) is a measure of verbal Ieaming and memory. The test involves five presentations (trials 1-5) of a shopping list consisting of 16 items from four different categories (tools, spices/herbs, clothing, and fruits), as well as one presentation of a list of distracter items. Most of the subscales within the CVLT consist of a total of 16 possible shopping items, and thus, performances across subscales are easily compared. The CVLT was designed to "quantify and provide normative data on how a Ieaming task is solved, that is, the different strategies, processes, and errors an examinee may display" (Delis, Kramer, Freedland, & Kaplan, 1988, pp.123- 124). The test evaluates concepts researched in cognitive psychology which have been found to distinguish different neurological populations. The CVLT assesses such variables as total recall, cued recall, semantic and serial learning strategies, serial position effects, Ieaming rates, consistency of item recall, proactive and retroactive interference, retention during short and long delays, perseverations and intrusions, and recognition. For the purposes of this study, the variables from the CVLT that were examined are total recall, recall after trial 1, recall after a short delay, recall after a long delay, cued recall after a short delay, cued recall after a long delay, semantic clustering, and recognition. A factor analysis of the CVLT was conducted by Delis, Kramer, Freedland, & Kaplan (1988) using the scores of 286 normal participants. This analysis revealed six factors which the authors termed general verbal Ieaming, 48 response discrimination, learning strategy, proactive effect, serial position effect, and acquisition rate. Relevant to the current study, the first factor includes the variables total recall, short-delay free recall, short-delay cued recall, long-delay free recall, long-delay cued recall, semantic clustering, and recognition. The third factor, Ieaming strategy, also includes the semantic clustering variable. The authors concluded that these results indicate that memory performance on the CVLT is represented by several theoretically meaningful factors. The numerous concepts and memory strategies represented by the CVLT variables make this instrument much more valuable than most memory tests which rely only on global recall scores. In addition, Delis, Kramer, Kaplan, and Ober (1987) reported that the CVLT total immediate recall across five trials had a correlation of .66 with the Wechsler Memory Scale (WMS) - Memory Quotient and correlations in the .60 range with many other WMS variables. Wechsler Memorv Scfiale (WMS; - Logical Memom Stories A and B of Logical Memory (LM) from the WMS (Wechsler, 1955; see Appendix E) served as a measure of text recall. For this study, only immediate recall of the stories was assessed, as LM was administered as part of the Senile Dementia Alzheimer's Type (SDAT) Battery which does not include the assessment of delayed recall (Storandt, Botwinick, Danziger, Berg, and Hughes, 1984). After being read each LM story, participants were required to immediately recall verbatim as much information as possible. The maximum score for each story, based on the total number of information units possible, is 49 22. A total score was then obtained based on performance on both of the LM stories. In several factor analytic studies, LM has consistently been found to load on the memory factor (Prigatano, 1978). The LM stories also have been found to have a significant negative relationship (I = -.38) with the Halstead-Reitan Average Impairment Scale among participants with brain damage (Russell, 1975). Results from clinical research indicate that LM is sensitive to memory deficits and characterizes memory disorders in various patient groups, such as those with dementia, multiple sclerosis, and depression (Spreen & Strauss, 1991). Procedure All participants in the experimental groups were assessed on the mental status measure, depression measures, and memory measures prior to (pretest) and after (posttest) receiving training sessions. Participants in the control group were tested approximately five weeks apart prior to receiving any training. Assignment to experimental and control groups was randomized. Pretest and posttest assessments were conducted at approximately a five week interval. Assessment sessions took approximately two hours. All tests used in the larger study were administered according to their standardized procedures. As stated earlier, the tests relevant to this study were administered as part of a more comprehensive cognitive assessment battery for a larger MSU Psychological Clinic aging project. Testing for the larger study was conducted by MSU clinical psychology doctoral students who have experience in the assessment of older 50 adults; these graduate students were blind to group assignment at pretest and posttest assessments. The tests were scored by each examiner, and then re— scored by a student responsible for data entry. The participants were not given monetary compensation for their involvement in this study; instead, they took part in workshops consisting of one of two types of memory training sessions. These individuals were also informed that they would be given feedback regarding their cognitive performance on the assessment measures. Testing was conducted individually with each participant, and testing sessions usually took place on the MSU campus in a comfortable, quiet location or at participants' homes. All training workshops were conducted on the MSU campus and took place twice per week for seven sessions. Training sessions, provided as part of the larger study, again were conducted by MSU clinical psychology doctoral students who had experience working with older adults. Workshops usually consisted of 5 - 10 group members and were led by the same clinician for seven sessions. Each session lasted for approximately 90 minutes. Instructors followed a training manual in order to conduct the seven session workshops (see below). Data from participants who attended less than five of the seven workshops were excluded from analyses. The experimental groups received memory training with either an additional attention training component or relaxation training component. Attention training techniques were similar to those described by van Zomeren 51 and Brouwer (1994); relaxation training was modeled after procedures developed by Benson, Beary, and Carol (1974). Memory training component that both experimental groups received began with educational instruction and discussion of age-related memory loss. The memory training consisted of teaching the method of loci technique and the face-name mnemonic technique, as well as discussing topics such as memory awareness and self-evaluation, memory and self-concept, self-change skills, pleasant activities and depression, positive thinking, and assertiveness. Educational handouts on all of these topics were provided. Reinforcement of the memory techniques included repetition of the practice tasks, positive feedback, and homework assignments. In addition to receiving the memory training described above, one experimental group received attention training as an additional training component which involved strategy and stimulation training. For the strategy attention training, guidelines were provided and discussed regarding preventing interruptions, avoiding time pressures, creating structure, and analyzing priorities. The stimulation attention training aspect involved practicing attention tasks, such as word searches, picking out missing details from pictures, and finding target letters/words among distracter items. The other experimental group received relaxation training as an additional component to the memory training. This additional training did not include any attention training exercises. Instead, these sessions included discussing the benefits of relaxation and practicing relaxation exercises. 52 As stated above, individuals in the control group did not receive any training until they had been administered a pretest and posttest at an interval of approximately five weeks. At that point, they were entitled to participate in the training workshops. The inclusion of their data in the current study did not depend on their workshop attendance. 53 RESULTS The groups for the present study consisted of participants who received [a] memory and attention training (p=40), [b] memory and relaxation training (p=32), and [c] no training (controls) (p=12). Demographic data for the groups are presented in Table 1. The three groups differed in their initial level of depression (E(2,81)=3.09, p=.05). Thus, initial level of depression was covaried in analyzing data. In addition, females performed significantly better than males on the majority of the verbal memory measures, consistent with findings in the literature (Hultsch, Hertzog, & Dixon, 1990; Schaie & Willis, 1993). Results were checked separately for males and females; however, no significant interactions were found between gender and group except for the CVLT first trial. Therefore, results are reported with men and women combined and the one relevant interaction is discussed below (Hypothesis 5). Any differences between the results from the combined group and the male and female groups is noted below. As the percentages of male and female participants differed between groups (see Table 1), analyses were conducted again with gender as a covariate; however, these results did not differ in significance from the prior results. No significant differences were found for the remaining demographic variables. The basic design for the present study is a three—group comparison of the pretest and posttest dependent variables. Thus, the multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) routine of SPSS (Version 8.0, 1997) was used as a 54 convenient way to conduct univariate analysis of variance (ANOVA) for the dependent variables. However, parts of certain hypotheses involved comparing test performance across all groups; therefore, paired-samples t-tests were used in these cases. Correlations among the dependent variables in the pretest data for the present study are presented in Table 2. Correlations among depression measures used and the pretest dependent variables are shown in Table 3. In addition, test-retest reliabilities of the dependent variables are presented in Table 4. The means and standard deviations of the dependent variables that were used in ANOVAs for all hypotheses are shown in Table 5. 55 Table 1 Demographic Characteristics of Memory and Attention (M+A)Training (Group A), Memory and Relaxation (M+R) Training (Group B), and Control Groups M+A Group M+R Group Controls E d_f p (E40) (0:32) (n=12) (Group A) (Group B) Gender (Frequencies) Males 15 16 5 Females 25 16 7 Age Mean 70.82 72.84 69.17 1.52 (2,81) .23 (SD) (6.92) (5.76) (8.67) Education Mean 16.35 16.50 16.75 .09 (2,81) .91 (SD) (2.75) (2.96) 3.25 Estimated Verbal IQ Mean 119.62 120.66 120.12 .17 (2,81) .85 (SD) (8.10) (7.00) (7.34) MMSE Mean 28.63 28.28 29.00 1.12 (2,81) .34 (SD) (1.39) (1.71) (1.21) Pretest BDI Mean 8.25 7.47 3.50 3.09 (2,81) .05 (SD) (7.38) (4.24) (2.58) Posttest BDI Mean 8.05 7.19 3.92 3.09 (2,80) .14 (SD) (7.65) (5.07) (2.54) (Table 1 continued) Note. MMSE = Mini-Mental State Examination; BDI = Beck Depression Inventory; GDS = Geriatric Depression Scale 56 Table 1 (continued) Demographic Characteristics of Memory and Attention (M+A)Training (Group A), Memory and Relaxation (M+R) Training (Group B), and Control Groups M+A Group M+R Group Controls E gt p (2:40) (fl=32) (0:12) (Group A) (Group B) Pretest GDS Mean 6.98 7.06 3.25 1.47 (2,80) .24 (SD) (6.71) (5.90) (3.11) Posttest GDS Mean 6.08 6.44 3.00 2.01 (2,81) .14 (SD) (6.79) (6.16) (2.30) Note. MMSE = Mini-Mental State Examination; BDI = Beck Depression Inventory; GDS = Geriatric Depression Scale 57 Table 2 Correlations among Pretest Dependent Variables 1.LM Total - 2.CVLT TR .54“ - 3.Trial 1 .38“ .66“ - 4.Short Free .33“ .76“ .44“ - 5.Short Cue .32" .77“ .38“ .87M - 6.Long Free .36“ .81“ .48“ .85“ .87“ - 7.Long Cue .35” .76“ .39" .85“ .88“ .89" 8.Seman CL .37“r .58" .45“ .54” .50“ .49" .47“ - 9.Recognitn .44" .37" .18 .44" .44“ .41" .53“ .34" - Note. All dependent variable p’s = 82 - 84; LM Total = Logical Memory Total Score; CVLT TR = CVLT Total Recall; Trial 1 = CVLT Trial 1; Short Free = CVLT Short-Delay Free Recall; Short Cue = CVLT Short-Delay Cued Recall; Long Free = CVLT Long-Delay Free Recall; Long Cue = CVLT Long-Delay Cued Recall; Seman CL = CVLT Semantic Cluster Ratio; Recognitn = CVLT Recognition Hits; “p<.001 58 Table 3 Correlations among Depression Measures and Pretest Dependent Variables BDI GDS BDI - GDS .86“ - LM Total -.07 -.09 CVLT TR .01 .00 Trial 1 -.14 -,14 Short Free -.05 -.05 Short Cue -.09 -.10 Long Free —.10 -.06 Long Cue -.01 .01 Seman CL -.04 -.03 Recognition .06 .06 Note. All dependent variable p’s = 82 - 84; BDI = Beck Depression Inventory; GDS = Geriatric Depression Scale; LM Total = Logical Memory Total Score; CVLT TR = CVLT Total Recall; Trial 1 = CVLT Trial 1; Short Free = CVLT Short-Delay Free Recall; Short Cue = CVLT Short-Delay Cued Recall; Long Free = CVLT Long-Delay Free Recall; Long Cue = CVLT Long-Delay Cued Recall; Seman CL = CVLT Semantic Cluster Ratio; Recognition = CVLT Recognition Hits; “p<.001 59 Table 4 Test-Retest Reliabilities of Dependent Variables (Pretest to Posttest) LMTotal .68“ CVLT TR .71 ** Trial 1 ,44" Short Free .63" Short Cue .69“ Long Free .68“ Long Cue .73” Seman CL .56“ Recognitn .31“ Note. All dependent variable p’s = 82 - 84; Row Headings = Pretest Dependent Variables; Column Headings = Posttest Dependent Variables; Pretest Dependent Variables: LM Total = Logical Memory Total Score; CVLT TR = CVLT Total Recall; Trial 1 = CVLT Trial 1; Short Free = CVLT Short-Delay Free Recall; Short Cue = CVLT Short-Delay Cued Recall; Long Free = CVLT Long-Delay Free Recall; Long Cue = CVLT Long- Delay Cued Recall; Seman CL = CVLT Semantic Cluster Ratio; Recognitn = CVLT Recognition Hits; Posttest Dependent Variables: 1.: Logical Memory Total Score; 2.= CVLT Total Recall; 3.= CVLT Trial 1; 4.= CVLT Short-Delay Free Recall; 5.= CVLT Short-Delay Cued Recall; 6.= CVLT Long-Delay Free Recall; 7.= CVLT Long-Delay Cued Recall; 8.= CVLT Semantic Cluster Ratio; 9.= CVLT Recognition Hits; “p<.001 6O The reliabilities shown in Table 4 are based on a test-retest period of approximately five weeks. The following are test-retest reliabilities that were available in the CVLT manual of CVLT scores after a one-year interval: Total Recall: r = .59”; Short-Delay Free Recall: r = .39; Short-Delay Cued Recall: r = .66°; Long-Delay Free Recall: r = .64°; Long-Delay Cued Recall: r = .79°; Semantic Cluster Ratio: r = .493; and Recognition Hits: r = .473 (ap<.05; bp<.01; °p<.001). Hypothesis 1 This hypothesis stated that participants in Group A (combined memory and attention training) would perform better on overall verbal memory measures (CVLT Total Recall and Logical Memory Total Score) than participants in Group B (combined memory and relaxation training). In addition, this hypothesis stated that both groups would perform better after training than participants in a control group that did not receive training. The hypothesis was tested by conducting two repeated measures ANOVA, each of which had one within-subjects factor with two levels (CVLT Total Recall'[pretest and posttest] and Logical Memory Total Score [pretest and posttest], respectively) crossed with one between-subjects factor (group). Prior to conducting the ANOVAs, CVLT Total Recall (pretest and posttest) and Logical Memory Total Score (pretest and posttest) were converted to z-scores. The time by group interactions for the dependent variables were nonsignificant (CVLT Total Recall, E(2,78) = .49, p=.62; Logical Memory Total Score, E(2,78) = .34, p=.72) (See Table 5); thus, Hypothesis 1 was not supported. Post-hoc repeated measures ANOVAs comparing the two 61 experimental groups without the smaller control group also revealed nonsignificant time by group interactions (CVLT Total Recall, E (1 ,67) = .00, p=.97; Logical Memory Total Score, E(1,67) = .61, p=.44). Conducting separate repeated measures ANOVAs for CVLT Total Recall and for Logical Memory Total Score did reveal a main effect for time for CVLT Total Recall (E(1,79) = 30.108, p<.001). Inspection of the means for all groups on CVLT Total Recall indicated that all groups improved from pretest to posttest (See Table 5) ; however, no significant time by group interaction was found. Hypothesis 2 This hypothesis stated that participants in Group A (combined memory and attention training) would demonstrate more active and organized recall strategies than participants in Group B (combined memory and relaxation training). Additionally, both groups would demonstrate more active and organized recall strategies than a control group. Hypothesis 2 was tested by conducting a repeated measures ANOVA with one 2 level within-subjects factor (pretest and posttest CVLT Semantic Clustering Ratio) crossed with one between-subjects factor (group). As shown in Table 5, a time by group interaction for the dependent variable was not significant (Semantic Clustering, E(2,79) = .31, p=.74), thus not providing support for this hypothesis. Results did reveal a main effect for time for semantic clustering (E(1,79) = 5.01, p<.05). Inspection of the means indicated that all groups improved from pretest to posttest. However, the relative contributions of males and females to this score differed. A repeated measures ANOVA for female participants revealed a main effect for Semantic 62 Clustering (_E(1,44) = 8.51, p<.05) indicating that the women’s performance across all groups improved over time; however, a repeated measures ANOVA for male participants did not reveal a main effect for this variable, E(1,31) = .27, p=.61. In addition, a post-hoc repeated measures ANOVA comparing the two experimental groups also revealed a nonsignificant time by group interaction, E(1,68) = .53, p=.47. Hypothesis 3 The first part of this hypothesis stated that all participants would perform better at pretest on a task of recognition memory (CVLT Recognition Hits) than on a task of free recall (CVLT Long-Delay Free Recall). Because these two variables are on the same scale (raw score out of 16 possible words), this part of the hypothesis was tested using a paired-samples t-test to compare all participants pretest performance on Recognition Hits to that on Long-Delay Free Recall. Results of the paired-samples t-test supported this portion of Hypothesis 3; scores were higher across all groups for recognition than free recall, 1(82) = 16.99, p<.001. The second part of this hypothesis stated that participants in Group A (combined memory and attention training) would perform better on free recall and recognition tasks (CVLT Long-Delay Free Recall and CVLT Recognition Hits) than participants in Group B (combined memory and relaxation training). Both groups were hypothesized to perform better on these measures than a control group. This portion of Hypothesis 3 was tested by conducting two repeated measures ANOVAs, each of which had one within-subjects factor with 63 two levels (CVLT Long-Delay Free Recall [pretest and posttest] and CVLT Recognition Hits [pretest and posttest], respectively) crossed with one between- subjects factor (group). Time by group interactions for the dependent variables were nonsignificant (CVLT Long-Delay Free Recall, _E(2,78) = 2.24, p=.11; CVLT Recognition Hits, E(2,78) = .08, p=.93) (See Table 5). Main effects were found for time for both dependent variables (CVLT Long-Delay Free Recall, E_(1,78) = 9.27, p<.05; CVLT Recognition Hits, _E(1,78) = 109.48, p<.001) indicating that all groups improved over time on both variables. A post-hoc repeated measures ANOVA comparing the two experimental groups without the smaller control group did reveal a significant time by group interaction for CVLT Long-Delay Free Recall, fi(1,67) = 4.55, p<.05; inspection of the means indicates that although both groups improved, as predicted Group A (combined memory and attention training) improved more than Group B (combined memory and relaxation training). Paired-samples t-tests revealed that Group A significantly improved from pretest to posttest on Long-Delay Free Recall, t(39) = 4.66, p<.001; whereas, Group B’s improvement was not statistically significant, 1(30) = 1.94, p=.06. Hypothesis 4 The first part of this hypothesis stated that all participants would benefit from information being presented with cues and would perform better on pretest cued recall than on unstructured word recall (CVLT Short-Delay Cued Recall vs. CVLT Short-Delay Free Recall; CVLT Long-Delay Cued Recall vs. CVLT Long- Delay Free Recall). Once again, because all of these variables are on the same 64 scale (raw score out of 16 possible words), this part of the hypothesis was tested using paired-samples t-tests. Comparisons were made between all participants pretest performance on [a] CVLT Short-Delay Cued Recall to that on CVLT Short-Delay Free Recall and on [b] CVLT Long-Delay Cued Recall to that on CVLT Long-Delay Free Recall. Results of the two paired-samples t-tests supported this portion of Hypothesis 4 (Short-Delay Cued Recall was better than Short-Delay Free Recall, t(82) = 7.89, p<.001; Long-Delay Cued Recall was better than Long-Delay Free Recall, 1(82) = 6.51, p<.001). The second part of Hypothesis 4 stated that participants who received combined memory and attention training (Group A) would perform better on free recall measures (CVLT Short-Delay Free Recall; CVLT Long-Delay Free Recall) than participants who received combined memory and relaxation training (Group B). In addition, this part of the hypothesis stated that both groups would perform better on these measures than a control group. Two repeated measures ANOVAs were conducted, each of which had one 2-level within-subjects factor (CVLT Short-Delay Free Recall [pretest and posttest] and CVLT Long-Delay Free Recall [pretest and posttest], respectively) crossed with one between- subjects factor (group). The time by group interactions for the dependent variables were nonsignificant (CVLT Short-Delay Free Recall, E(2,79) = .33, p=.72; CVLT Long-Delay Free Recall, E(2,79) = .20, p=.82) (See Table 5). Results did reveal main effects for time for both dependent variables (CVLT Short-Delay Free Recall, E(1,79) = 7.69, p<.01; CVLT Long-Delay Free Recall, _E(1,79) = 4.44, p<.05); inspection of the means indicated that all groups 65 improved from pretest to posttest on both variables. A post-hoc repeated measures ANOVA comparing the two experimental groups revealed nonsignificant time by group interactions for both CVLT Short-Delay (_E(1,68) = .77, p=.38) and Long-Delay (E(1,68) = .01, p=.91). Thus, the second part of Hypothesis 4 was not supported. Hypothesis 5 This hypothesis stated that participants in Group A (combined memory and attention training) would perform better on unstructured immediate word recall (CVLT Trial 1) than participants in Group B (combined memory and relaxation training). Again, both groups were hypothesized to perform better on unstructured immediate word recall after training than a control group. Hypothesis 5 was tested by conducting a repeated measures ANOVA with one 2 level within-subjects factor (pretest and posttest CVLT Trial 1) crossed with one between-subjects factor (group). The time by group interaction for Trial 1 was nonsign'rficant, E(2,78) = 1.08, p=.34. However, there was a main effect for time for Trial 1 (_E(1,78) = 18.39, p<.001) because all groups improved from pretest to posttest. Results from conducting a post-hoc ANOVA comparing the two experimental groups also revealed a nonsignificant time by group interaction for Trial 1, E(1,68) = 1.56, p=.22. A repeated measures ANOVA for male participants did reveal a marginally significant time by group interaction for Trial 1, _F_(2,31) = 3.3, p=.05; however, results of paired-samples t-tests revealed that the only significant pretest to posttest improvement in Trial 1 recall occurred for male control participants, 1(4) = 5.10, p<.01. It is noteworthy that the sample 66 consisted of only five male control participants. No significant difference in Trial 1 recall existed between the male groups at pretest (E(2,31) = 2.31, p=.12) or at posttest, _F_(2,31) = .12, p=.89. Thus, based on all of these results, this hypothesis was not supported. 67 Table 5 Univariate Analyses of Variance for Dependent Variables (with BDI as a Covariate) M+A Group M+R Group Controls _E Q; p (Group A) (Group 8) LM Total (Hyp. 1) Pretest: Mean 19.97 17.13 17.50 (SD) (5.71) (5.80) (5.73) Posttest: Mean 20.58 18.63 18.75 (SD) (6.44) (6.29) (5.97) Time Main Effect .77 (1,78) .38 Group Main Effect 2.37 (2,78) .10 Time x Group .34 (2,78) .72 CVLT TR (Hyp. 1) Pretest; Mean 44.48 42.87 41.25 (SD) (9.26) (8.46) (10.91) Posttest: Mean 50.88 47.94 50.75 (SD) (12.04) (9.89) (11.99) Time Main Effect 30.11 (1,79) .00 Group Main Effect .71 (2,79) .50 Time x Group 1.36 (2,79) .26 Seman CL (Hyp. 2) Pretest; Mean 1.71 1.70 1.75 (SD) (0.71) (0.84) (0.92) Posttest; Mean 2.03 2.20 2.14 (SD) (1.07) (0.93) (1.05) Time Main Effect 5.01 (1,79) .03 Group Main Effect .09 (2,79) .92 Time x Group .31 (2,79) .74 Long Free (Hyp. 3) Pretest: Mean 8.20 8.71 8.17 (SD) (3.01) (2.87) (2.82) Posttest: Mean 10.13 9.63 10.08 (SD) (3.42) (3.56) (3.58) Time Main Effect 9.27 (1,78) .00 Group Main Effect .01 (2,78) .99 Time x Group 2.24 (2,78) .11 Recognitn (Hyp. 3) Pretest Mean 13.55 13.97 13.50 (SD) (2.23) (1.89) (3.03) Posttest; Mean 14.63 14.32 13.83 (SD) (1.72) (1.59) (2.55) Time Main Effect 109.4 (1.78) -00 Group Main Effect -33 (2.78) 72 Time x Group .08 (2,78) .93 (Table 5 continued) 68 Table 5 (continued) Univariate Analyses of Variance for Dependent Variables M+A Group M+R Group Controls E _d_f p (Group A) (Group B) Short Free (Hyp. 4) Pretest Mean 8.05 7.94 8.08 (SD) (3.62) (2.95) (2.87) Posttest: Mean 9.60 9.56 8.92 (SD) (3.31) (3.63) (3.63) Time Main Effect 7.69 (1 ,79) .01 Group Main Effect .08 (2,79) .93 Time x Group .33 (2,79) .72 Short Cue (Hyp. 4) (paired-samples t-test) Pretest: Mean 9.35 9.45 9.42 (SD) (3.13) (2.35) (3.29) Posttest: Mean 10.85 10.78 1 1.50 (SD) (2.82) (3.09) (2.68) Long Free (Hyp. 4) Pretest: Mean 8.20 8.71 8.17 (SD) (3.01) (2.87) (2.82) Posttest: Mean 10.13 9.63 10.08 (SD) (3.42) (3.56) (3.58) Time Main Effect 4.44 (1 ,79) .04 Group Main Effect .01 (2,79) .99 Time x Group .20 (2,79) .82 Long Cue (Hyp. 4) (paired-samples t-test) Pretest: Mean 9.30 9.55 9.17 (SD) (3.07) (3.00) (2.69) Posttest: Mean 10.83 10.63 11.42 (SD) (2.96) (3.10) (2.88) Trial 1 (Hyp. 5) Pretest: Mean 6.05 5.65 5.67 (SD) (1.43) (1.56) (2.31) Posttest: Mean 7.58 6.69 7.45 (SD) (2.66) (1.67) (2.25) Time Main Effect 18.39 (1,78) .00 Group Main Effect 2.05 (2,78) .14 Time x Group 1-03 (2.78) -34 Note. LM Total = Logical Memory Total Score; CVLT TR = CVLT Total Recall; Seman CL = CVLT Semantic Cluster Ratio; Long Free = CVLT Long-Delay Free Recall; Recognitn = CVLT Recognition Hits; Short Free = CVLT Short-Delay Free Recall; Short Cue = CVLT Short-Delay Cued Recall; Long Cue = CVLT Long-Delay Cued Recall; Trial 1 = CVLT Trial 1 69 DISCUSSION The overall objectives of the present study were to examine the effects of memory/attention training and memory/relaxation training on the verbal memory abilities of healthy older adults. In general, it was predicted that participants who received combined memory and attention training would perform better on verbal memory measures than those who received combined memory and relaxation training. Furthermore, it was predicted that participants in both experimental groups would perform better than participants in a control group. The results of the present study provide some support for the prediction that memory and attention training would have more beneficial effects than memory and relaxation training on the verbal memory abilities of healthy older adults. Specifically, this prediction was supported by way of participants in Group A (combined memory and attention training) performing better after training on Long-Delay Free Recall than participants in Group B (combined memory and relaxation training). However, given the post-hoc nature of this finding, it needs to be regarded with some tentativeness at best. All other predictions regarding the differential effects of combined memory and attention training, combined memory and relaxation training, and no training were not supported. Specifically, interactions between time and group for the dependent variables (Logical Memory Total Score, CVLT Total Recall, Semantic Clustering Ratio, Recognition Hits, Short-Delay Free Recall, Long-Delay Free Recall, and CVLT Trial 1) were not significant. Thus, the overall evidence from the present study for the 70 effectiveness of combined memory and attention training and combined memory and relaxation training is weak. In the realm of the overall verbal memory abilities of healthy older adults, the prediction that healthy older adults would perform better on a task involving recognition memory than on a task involving unstructured free recall was supported. This was represented by all participants performing better at pretest on CVLT Recognition Hits than on CVLT Long-Delay Free Recall. Along these lines, the prediction that healthy older adults’ verbal memory abilities would improve after being presented with cues was also supported. Specifically, participants in all groups performed better on CVLT Short-Delay Cued Recall than on CVLT Short-Delay Free Recall and performed better on CVLT Long- Delay Cued Recall than on CVLT Long-Delay Free Recall. These results are discussed in turn below for each hypothesis. The first prediction was that participants who received combined memory and attention training would perform better than participants who received combined memory and relaxation training on overall verbal memory measures, as represented by scores on Logical Memory and CVLT Total Recall, and that both groups would perform better than a control group. This was not supported. No significant interaction was found between time and group for Logical Memory or CVLT Total Recall. Based on findings in the literature that elderly individuals can experience difficulties in effortful processes requiring higher levels of attention (Hasher & Zacks, 1979; Jennings & Jacoby, 1993; Light & Singh, 1987), in selective attention abilities (Farkas & Hoyer, 1980; Madden, 1983; 71 McDowd & Birren, 1990; Plude & Hoyer, 1986), and in inhibitory attentional processes (Hasher & Zacks, 1988; Kane et al., 1994; West & Bell, 1997), it was proposed that adding an attention-training component to memory training would more greatly enhance older adults’ memory abilities than would combining relaxation and memory training. However, results indicated that all participants, regardless of group assignment, significantly improved from pretest to posttest in their CVLT Total Recall performance, but did not significantly improve in their Logical Memory performance. The improvement of all participants, including the individuals in the control group, in CVLT Total Recall may be the result of a nonspecific effect of simply being included in a study designed to improve memory. This may be similar to the medical placebo effect which can aid in or cause recovery from illness and disease. Recently, placebo effects have not only been to shown to be caused by pharrnacologically inactive agents but also by elements of the patient-physician encounter, such as being told when recovery from an illness will occur, being given expectations regarding the powerful effects of medicines, and receiving diagnostic tests (Brown, 1998; Sox, Margulies, & Sox, 1981; Thomas, 1987). Thus, perhaps being contacted by experimenters involved with a study designed to improve memory and receiving a battery of tests designed to assess memory abilities can improve memory functioning. On the other hand, the improvement of all participants in CVLT Total Recall may reflect practice effects, as the interval of time between the administration of the pretest and posttest was only approximately four to six weeks. Performance 72 on the CVLT may be more susceptible to practice effects than Logical Memory as the CVLT involves the immediate recall of a list of words for each of five trials, recall of these words after both a short and long delay, and recognition of these words in a final trial; whereas, Logical Memory for this protocol entails only the immediate recall of a short paragraph that is presented once. The magnitude of practice effects associated with the CVLT and with Logical Memory are not directly compared in the literature, although recent findings do indicate that significant practice effects occur for both of these tests (Rapport, Axelrod, Theisen, Brines, Kalechstein, & Ricker, 1997). The results regarding Logical Memory may differ between the present study and the Rapport et al. (1997) study due to the latter study’s use of a newer version of Logical Memory stories from the WMS-R and to the inclusion of delayed recall for Logical Memory. Requiring participants to recall the Logical Memory stories after a 30-minute delay, in addition to requiring immediate recall of the stories, may have enhanced practice effects in the Rapport et al. (1997) study. Findings from another recent study also revealed a moderate practice effect on Logical Memory from the WMS-R for college students (Dikmen, Heaton, Grant, and Temkin; 1999). Furthermore, Dikmen et al. reported that practice effects tend to be greater for individuals who are younger and have higher levels of education. Thus, perhaps the older age of the participants in the present study limited the effects of practice for Logical Memory, but their high level of education contributed to possible practice effects for the CVLT. 73 The second hypothesis, that participants in Group A (combined memory and attention training) would demonstrate more active, organized recall strategies, as represented by the CVLT Semantic Clustering Ratio, than participants in Group B (combined memory and relaxation training) and that both groups would demonstrate more active, organized recall strategies than those in a control group was not supported. Instead, initial results indicated that all participants improved from pretest to posttest in their ability to use active, organized recall strategies. Examination of the relative contributions of males and females to this score revealed that females across all groups improved in their semantic clustering abilities over time; whereas, the males did not improve. The finding that women were able to improve their semantic clustering strategies supports the reports in the literature that females tend to have more intact verbal memory abilities as they age as compared to males (Hultsch et al., 1990; Schaie & Willis, 1993) and corresponds to the finding in the present study that females performed better than males across all verbal memory measures. However, memory training did not have any greater effect on using active recall strategies than simply being included in a control group that received no training. Once again, improvement in semantic clustering ability over time may be a result of nonspecific effects of being included in a study of memory abilities and/or practice effects of the CVLT. The first prediction for Hypothesis 3 that all participants would perform better at pretest on a task of recognition memory, as represented by CVLT Recognition Hits, than on a task of free recall, as represented by CVLT Long- 74 Delay Free Recall, was confirmed by the data. All participants scored higher on recognition than on free recall. These results support the findings in the literature that older adults tend to perform more poorly on measures of recall than on those of recognition and that deficits in the ability to process information at a deep level may contribute to free recall difficulties (Craik & Lockhart, 1972; Craik & McDowd, 1987; Craik, 1994). Deficits in older adults’ deeper processing abilities may cause retrieving words from memory with no cues to become more difficult than simply recognizing the words. Again this is similar to Hasher and Zacks’ (1979) theory that older adults have deficits in effortful processing abilities. The second prediction for Hypothesis 3 was that participants who received combined memory and attention training would perform better on free recall and recognition tasks (CVLT Long-Delay Free Recall and Recognition Hits, respectively) than participants who received combined memory and relaxation training, and that both groups would perform better on these measures than a control group. Initial findings revealed that participants in all groups improved over time on both free recall and recognition. However, a post-hoc comparison of the two experimental groups without the smaller control group revealed that although both groups improved in their free recall (Long-Delay), participants who received combined memory and attention training did improve more than those who received memory and relaxation training. The above finding indicates, specifically, that the long-delay recall abilities of healthy older adults are enhanced by practicing attention-related tasks when 75 Ieaming memory improvement techniques; although, due to the post-hoc nature of this finding, it needs to be interpreted very tentatively. At the same time, this result is consistent with the theory that decreases in attentional capacity as adults age affects the efficiency and use of effortful processes, thus contributing to memory deficits (Craik & McDowd, 1987; Hasher & Zacks, 1979; Light & Singh, 1987; Titov & Knight, 1997). Furthermore, the current finding is somewhat similar to Neely and Backman’s (1995) results that attention training combined with the method of loci training improved older adults’ recall ability; however, these authors included a relaxation-training component in their attention/method of loci training. Thus, it is difficult to distinguish between the effects of attention training and relaxation training in the Neely and Backman study; whereas, the present finding does tentatively reveal one differential effect of attention and relaxation training. Although a post-hoc distinction was made between the two experimental groups, it should be noted that significant differences between the performance of the experimental and control groups were not found. The first prediction for Hypothesis 4 that all participants would benefit from information being presented with cues was supported. Participants across all groups performed better at pretest on cued recall tasks (CVLT Short-Delay Cued Recall and Long-Delay Cued Recall) than on free recall tasks (CVLT Short-Delay Free Recall and Long-Delay Free Recall). These cued recall tasks involved the presentation of cues for semantic categories. As older adults have been reported to have deficits in their ability to actively categorize words (Burke & 76 Light, 1981; Dobbs et al., 1989), the present finding indicates that providing cues to aid in semantic categorization enhances the memory abilities of older adults. The second prediction for Hypothesis 4 stated that participants in Group A (combined memory and attention training) would perform better on free recall measures (CVLT Short-Delay Free Recall and Long-Delay Free Recall) than participants in Group B (combined memory and relaxation training), and that both groups would perform better on these measures than participants in a control group. This prediction was not confirmed by the data. Results indicated that participants across all groups improved over time on both free recall measures. Neither the combined memory and attention training nor the combined memory and relaxation training seemed to have any greater effect on older adults’ verbal memory abilities than simply being included in this study in a control group. The final prediction that participants who received combined memory and attention training would perform better on unstructured immediate word recall (CVLT Trial 1) than participants who received combined memory and relaxation training was not supported In addition, the expectation that both groups would perform better on this measure than participants in a control group was not confirmed. All participants, regardless of group, improved over time in their unstructured immediate word recall. Once again, the nonspecific effects of being included in this study and/or the practice effects associated with the CVLT may have contributed to the improvement across groups. Upon examination of the relative contributions of males versus females to the Trial 1 scores, an unexpected marginally significant interaction between time and group on Trial 1 77 for males was found. Male participants in the control group exhibited greater improvement in their unstructured immediate word recall than either male participants in Group A (combined memory and attention training) or male participants in Group B (combined memory and relaxation training). However, this likely is a result of small sample size; the male control group consisted of only five participants. It is noteworthy that the control group in the present study consisted of only 12 participants. The small size of this group (relative to the 40 and 32 participants in the memory/attention training and memory/relaxation training groups, respectively) may have contributed to the lack of distinction between the experimental and control conditions. Furthermore, it was more difficult to recruit participants for the control group than for the experimental groups as the control participants were not allowed to take part in the memory training groups until they had completed both the pretest and posttest. Some individuals refused to take part in this study as a result of having to wait to participate in the training groups until they had completed two testing sessions. Therefore, the individuals that did agree to participate in the control group may have been highly self- selected in their level of motivation. This may have positively affected their test performance in that these participants may have been more motivated to perform their best during memory testing. In addition, the control participants reported experiencing fewer depressive symptoms than participants in the experimental groups (see Table 1). Thus, individuals in the control group may 78 have been more able to perform to the best of their ability as a result of experiencing better moods and/or fewer somatic difficulties. Overall, these results tentatively indicate that combined memory and attention training more effectively enhances the specific ability of healthy older adults to recall words after a long delay than combined memory and relaxation training. However, the experimental groups did not recall words better after a long delay than the control group. Additionally, combined memory and attention training does not seem to have a greater effect on overall verbal memory, semantic clustering, immediate recall, short-delay recall, cued recall, or recognition abilities of older adults than does combined memory and relaxation training nor than does inclusion in a control group. In general, these results seem to indicate that memory/attention training and memory/relaxation training techniques were relatively ineffective. It should be noted, however, that the effects of memory/attention and memory/relaxation training may have been difficult to detect due to the small size of the control group. Regarding overall verbal memory abilities, as predicted able elderly perform better on tasks involving the recognition of verbal information as opposed to the free recall of this information. Furthermore, older adults’ verbal memory abilities are enhanced by the presentation of cues that aid in the categorical organization of verbal material. These results highlight the importance of including control groups in treatment outcome studies; without the inclusion of a control group in the present study, it would appear that both types of memory training improved the verbal 79 memory abilities of healthy older adults. Specifically, the improvement that occurred over time across all groups in overall verbal memory (CVLT Total Recall), immediate recall (CVLT Trial 1), semantic clustering for women, delayed free recall (CVLT Short-Delay Free Recall; CVLT Long-Delay Free Recall), and recognition would appear to have been the result of the cognitive retraining techniques when in fact, the improvement may have been caused by CVLT practice effects and/or the nonspecific effects of being included in this study. It is interesting to note that most of the cognitive retraining studies in the literature have not employed control groups (Anschutz et al., 1985; Baltes & Kliegl, 1992; Gratzinger et al., 1990; Kliegl et al., 1989; Rockstroh et al., 1995; Willis et al., 1983; Yesavage, 1984; Yesavage & Jacob, 1984; Yesavage & Rose, 1983; Yesavage & Rose, 1984a; Yesavage & Rose, 1984b) and thus, findings from these studies that memory and/or attention training improves cognitive abilities may be in question. Two studies in the literature which did employ control groups have similarities and differences to the current study (Yesavage et al., 1983; Neely & Backman, 1995). As discussed previously, Yesavage et al. (1983) compared the recall abilities of elderly adults who were trained to use the face-name mnemonic technique combined with making affective judgments of their visual image association, those who were trained only in the face-name mnemonic technique, and elderly adults who did not receive training. Both of the experimental groups performed better than the control group; however, the interaction was not significant. The present study did employ the face-name mnemonic technique; 80 however, training also included the method of loci imagery technique in addition to either attention or relaxation training. Thus, it is somewhat difficult to compare the results of the two studies because different training techniques were used as commonly occurs in the memory/attention training literature. Also, Yesavage et al. (1983) used recall of names as the outcome measure in their study. As the face-name mnemonic technique is intended to directly improve recall of names, this likely contributed to Yesavage et al.’s findings. Neely and Backman (1995) investigated the effects of attention, memory, and relaxation training on older adults’ verbal memory. Similar to the present study, attention training and the method of loci techniques were used in an effort to improve recall of words; however, relaxation training was also included in the one experimental group. Additionally, the face-name mnemonic technique was not employed. Neely and Backman found that that participants who received the combined training improved in their word recall; whereas, control participants did not improve. The authors conceded that it is unclear which of the training techniques contributed to their findings since all of the techniques were combined for the experimental group. Once again, this makes it difficult to compare the Neely and Backman results to the present findings, although comparison of the pattern of effect sizes across groups from each of these studies might shed light on the conflicting findings. If the patterns of data across groups are similar, then greater support is provided for the notion that the small size of the control group in the present study may have contributed to finding several nonsignificant results. 81 The sample used for the present study was a relatively homogenous group of individuals. Participants were Caucasian, middle class individuals from the Mid-Michigan area. Furthermore, 93% of the participants spoke English as their first language, and 85% of the participants had held professional or managerial jobs. Also, the mean level of education and estimated verbal IQ for this sample was well above average and the variance for each of these variables was low (years education: mean = 16.46, standard deviation = 2.87; estimated verbal IQ: mean = 120.09, standard deviation = 7.52 [See Table 1 for means and standard deviations by group]). The inclusion of faculty retiree groups from MSU in this sample likely contributed to the high mean level of education and estimated verbal IQ. The lack of variability among participants may have contributed to the limited findings in the present study. Thus, it should be noted that the present findings may not generalize beyond highly educated, intelligent, Caucasian, healthy older adults. One limitation of the present study may be that the outcome measures (CVLT and Logical Memory) used to evaluate memory improvement do not adequately assess real-world positive changes that may have occurred as a result of training. For instance, perhaps participants who received combined memory and attention training improved more than participants who received combined memory and relaxation training in their ability to remember where they placed items in their homes and/or what their intended purpose was for entering a particular room. Both groups may have improved more than the control group in these areas. However, it is difficult to create objective measures of such 82 improvement. The present study focused on the verbal memory abilities of elderly adults as these individuals frequently complain of verbal memory difficulties, such as not remembering what others have just said (Kausler, 1985), and as objective measures of these abilities exist. An additional limitation of the current study may be that correlations among certain CVLT dependent variables were high (See Table 2). Correlations between Short-Delay Free Recall, Short-Delay Cued Recall, Long-Delay Free Recall, and Long-Delay Cued Recall ranged between .85 and .89. This may indicate that these dependent variables measure the same construct. Although a significant interaction between time and group for Long-Delay Free Recall was found for the memory/attention training and memory/relaxation training groups, finding nonsignificant interactions between time and group for the remaining highly correlated variables may be a result of repeatedly measuring the same construct. Thus, finding several nonsignificant results in this study may not represent as strong evidence against the notion that memory training can enhance memory abilities as would obtaining numerous nonsignificant findings for entirely different variables. Future research in cognitive retraining of able older adults may benefit from looking more closely at the effects of emotional factors, such as depression and anxiety. As depression has been shown to negatively impact the memory abilities of older adults (Burt et al., 1995; LaRue, 1992; Weingartner et al., 1981), the present study did control for depression during data analysis; however, this study did not measure nor control the particpants’ level of anxiety. Thus, future 83 studies may benefit from controlling for anxiety as an additional variable. Further exploration of the relationship between these emotional factors and memory training may help to clarify research findings. Additionally, it is strongly recommended that future research examining cognitive retraining of older adults include control groups to evaluate the effectiveness of the different techniques used. It is also important to employ control groups in an attempt to replicate the findings from previous memory and attention training studies. Additionally, the use of larger sample sizes, particularly regarding the control group in the present study, may be helpful in obtaining more numerous significant findings. It may also be beneficial to use verbal memory measures that have alternate forms so as to avoid the potential practice effects of the CVLT. Moreover, future research should investigate the effects of cognitive retraining techniques on memory using a more heterogeneous sample so that future findings would more likely generalize to a larger segment of healthy older adults in the general population. 84 APPENDICES 85 APPENDIX A Beck Depression Inventory (Beck et al., 1961) I Date: Name: Marital Status: __ Age: Sex: Occupation: Education: This questionnaire consists of 21 groups of statements. After reading each group of statements carefully, circle the number (0, 1. 2 or 3) next to the one statement in each group which best describes the way you have been feeling the past week. including today. If several statements within a group seem to apply equally well. circle each one. Be sure to read all the statements in each group before making your choi ice. I do not feel sad. on 't ee am an worse than I I I d r 1 I y I feel sad. anyb°dy else . . . I am critical of myself for my weaknesses I am sad all the tune and I can t snap out of it. or mistakes I am so sad or unhappy that I can t stand it. I blame myself all the'time for my mung 2 I am not particularly discouraged about the gaifigpgggu for everything bad uture. I feel discouraged about the future. 9 I don't have any thoughts of 1d . g myself. I feel I have nothing to look forward to. I have thou hts of killin , I feel that the future 15 hopeless and that g g myself bin 1 would not carry them out. things cannot improve I would like to kill myself. 3 Idonotfeellikeafailure. Iwouldkillmyselfiflhad the chance. ' I feel I have failed more than the average person. III I don't cry any more than usual. As I look back on my life. all I can see is I cry more now than I used to. a lot of failures. I cry an the time now. I 18911 8111 a complete failure as a Person- I used to be able to cry. but now I can 't cry even though I want to. 4 I get as much satisfaction out of things as I used ‘to. _ . 11 I am no more irritated now than I ever am. I don t enjoy things the way I used to. I get annoyed or irritated more easily than I don't get real satisfaction out of anything I used to. anymore. I feel irritated all the time now. I don‘t get irritated at all by the things that used to irritate me. I am dissatisfied or bored with everything. 5 I don't feel particularly guilty. I 1991 guilty a 800d 133-?t 0f the 1:1va 12 I have not lost interest in other people. I feel quite guilty most of the time. I am less interested in other people than I feel guilty all of the time. I used to be. I have lost most of my interest in 3 I don‘t feel I am being punished. other people. I feel I mav be punished. I have lost all of my interest in Other people. I expect to be punished. I feel I am being punished. 13 I make decisions about as well as I ever could. . . - - , I put off making decisions more than 7 I don tlfeel disappointed in my self. I used to I am disappomted m myself. I have greater difficulty in making I am disgusted With myself. decisions than before. I hate myself. I can't make decisions at all anymore. Subtotal Page 1 CONTINUED ON BACK 86 14 IS 17 13 I don't feel I look any worse than I used to. I am worried that I am looking old or unattraCtive. I feel that there are permanent changes in my appearance that make me look unattractive. I believe that I look ugly. I can work about as well as before. It takes an extra effort to get started at doing something. I have to push myself very hard to do anvthing. I can't do any work at all. I can sleep as well as usual. I don't sleep as well as I used to. I wake up 1 -2 hours earlier than usual and find it hard to get back to sleep. . I wake up several hours earlier than I used to and cannot get back to sleep. I don't get more tired than usual. I get tired more easily than I used to. I get tired from doing almost anything. I am too tired to do anything. My appetite is no worse than usual. My appetite is noc as good as it used to be. My appetite is much worse now. I have no appetite at all anymore. 15 21 I haven‘t lost much weight, if any, lately. I have lost more than 5 pounds. I have lost more than 10 pounds. I have lost more than 1 5 pounds. I am purposely trying to lose weight by eating less. Yes No I am no more worried about my health than usual. I am worried about physical problems such as aches and pains: or upset stomach; or constipation. I am very worried about physical problems and it's hard to think of much else. I am so worried about my physical problems that I cannot think about anything else. I have not noticed any recent change in my tnterest in sex. I am less interested in sex than I used to be. I am much less interested in sex now. I have lost interest in sex completely. 87 Subtotal Page 2 Subtotal Page 1 Total Score APPENDIX B Geriatric Depression Scale (Yesavage et al., 1983) GDS Name: Date: Total Score: Clinician: 10. 11. 12. l3. 14. Choose the best answer for how you felt over the past week Are you basically satisfied with your life? ..... Yes/No Have you dropped many of your activities and interests? ..... Yes/No Do you feel that your life is empty?....Yes/No Do you often get bored? ..... Yes/No Are you hopeful about the future? ..... Yes/No Are you bothered by thoughts you can’t get out of your head? ..... Yes/No Are you in good spirits most of the time? ..... Yes/No Are you afraid that something bad is going to happen to you? ..... Yes/No Do you feel happy most of the time? ..... Yes/No Do you feel helpless? ..... Yes/No - Do you often get restless and fidgety? ..... Yes/No Do you prefer to stay at home, rather than going out and doing new things? ..... Yes/No Do you frequently worry about the future? ..... Yes/No Do you feel you have more problems with memory than most? ..... Yes/No 88 15. 16. 17. 18. 19. 27. 28. 29. 30. Do you think it is wonderful to be alive now? ..... Yes/No Do you often feel downhearted and blue? ..... Yes/No Do you feel pretty worthless the way you are now? ..... Yes/No Do you worry a lot about the past? ..... Yes/No Do you find life very exciting? ..... Yes/No Is it hard for you to get started on new projects? ..... Yes/No Do you feel full of energy? ..... Yes/No Do you feel that your situation is hopeless? ..... Yes/No Do you think that most people are better off than you are? ..... Yes/No Do you frequently get upset over little things? ..... Yes/No Do you frequently feel like crying? ..... Yes/No Do you have trouble concentrating? ..... Yes/No Do you enjoy getting up in the morning? ..... Yes/No Do you prefer to avoid social gatherings?.....Yes/No Is it easy for you to make decisions? ..... Yes/No Is your mind as clear as it used to be? ..... Yes/No 89 APPENDIX C Mini-Mental State Examination (Folstein et al., 1975) B . o Canyoualsotallmdwwmdmishospitaudhfic)? mamas) Whafloormwoon? TFIoor. City mewnordtymwoh? . ounce magma-min? may Matmmwoh? » ' WW— Asktha 'ectifyoumtoahis/hetmm._Thmsay'bar.1ag'.. ‘aall' w'demwdaudymbanomsecmdfamn Meryoulnvesaud 'fiag: ammm/wwmmmmmdm “I'm _Iis/hersepre(0~1).bukaepsnyit\gMuflho/shoennrepmal3. uptosu'ies. Ifhe/slndoesnotavmmlyleamaflareaflm d' ‘ beWytwed. . ' Nunba'. tnals: Ill ATT‘NTI N Tl N7 8 mmw‘mmWM1mwmfiww . top -dtcr5wba!acflau(93.86.79.72.65). mmmm deonectm "unmeamorwinotpcflommmww wm/wwwmmwm flusccrosmnunber dmhmcxada. Famdedmbadlwaa. " NW mmesubjeatoreaflmeawocdsyoupmioudyukad rim/hate camber. Senna-3 V-JAW gé ggg idadqqaaqq WsmNinwiwammwukM/wmkk Repeatiorpmd. REPETmou Askmastbieettonpm‘Nohandaorbtn' TalmWhn-igum 3-STAGE COMMAND . . . Fddsm ”a” Gmmesubioctapioccdplanblankpaperarusyfirakounpapernyw Puts pa cancer www.mnhwandpmtonmm: paper READING . F'M'Ctosoywrms'sheet. Askhim/hertomdtanddowhatltsays. Scarceomctodyilho/smactuaflydosahis/hereyas. Oceans: GMNufesubieaablaMpiccadpapet askfim/Mrmwriteam Itis mum-41W. nmmmasubicctandamwbem Camctgtamnarwpumdonmnotneccswy. Wm _— COPYiNG Ask 'cctto NW‘WM.AI10WMNW . wgmmmWWMTmemm Drawspemagons __ « <31: oemvme Tom _* - ‘3 Smmenumberofconectrepliestothetestituns. Itammspaledwwasgedmmww correct letters given in propersequence (oneto five). Thermnmum scoresao form: TOTAL SCORE: 90 APPENDIX D California Verbal Learning Test (Delis et al., 1987) LIST A: Immediate Free Recall, Trials 13 Instructions to Examinee: Triall: Let ‘s suppose you were going shopping on Monday. I 'm going to read a list of items for you to buy. Listen carefully. and when I '12! through. I wantyou to say back as many of the items asyou can. It doesn 't matter what orderyou say them in —just tell me as manyasyou can. Are you ready? Trial 2: I 'm going to repeat Monday‘s shopping list. Again. I want you to say back as many items as you can. in any order. Be sure to also say the items on the list thatyou told me the first time. Trials 35: I‘m going to repeat Monday's shopping list. Again. I want you to say back as many items as you can. in any order: including items you may have already told me. KEY FOR CODING RESPONSE TYPE C a Correct 1? = Perseveration I = Intrusion chunllc Clultlr Smnunlln Chill" 1 3 '5 3 o 3 5 E U I 91 LIST A: Immediate Free Recall. Trials 4 8:. 5 '2 .2 Table of i 5 5: Free-Recall , i g 3 Word Order h E : C E l ? E 2 '6 a E '6 a a m a: LIST A E 2' 2 List 1 2 . 3 : 4 l 5 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 1 1 12 1 3 14 1 5 16 1 7 18 19 Serial cluster I i I ' score: 1 . | Summary Table: LIST A (Monday List) Table of Serial Position Recall: Immediate Free Recall LIST A Immediate Free Recall, 2 I Pvtuver- ! l Scmanuc m 1'5 TRIAL l Correct (C) I ationslP) l Intrusionul) ! Cluster ‘I Number 1 ‘ g I t I Remon I Comet ' of Total x l 5 i l 3 ; E ’ Primacy ' n ' i l : ' i ‘ l ' t 1 l I g ( 3 Middle ' l 3 r a a : l i I 3 E ' l l l Recent)! l 4 ! i . ! m ! ' :oow. . l ; l : TOTAL 3 l ! 3 W Record these totals in the Summon-5~ ‘I‘able of Recall Errors (p 9) 92 LIST B (Tuesday List) Instructions: 'ow let's suppose that you planned to go shopping again on Tuesday. I 'm going to read a new list of items for you to buy. When I‘m through. I want you to say back as many asyou can. in any order: LIST A: Short-Delay Free Recall Instructions: Now I 'd like you to tell me all of the shoppingitems you can from the Alonday list. Perseverations (P): Intrusions (I): Semantic cluster score: Serial cluster score: Perseverations (P): Intrusions (I): Semantic cluster score: l : 5 e . c o .5 LIST B l 2 Responses g Responses 9:; (Tuesday) ,3. E 3 List 3 3 .. ,' ~.".‘_. 51:71:;- 1’.’ . toasterssz-sr. :é‘r. 1 .- -. 13mg ‘— .’~.A.—-.. cherries 233951' 2 2 '11" v; #.',‘2;;Jj..r::.r.:.5 halibut-'2- =~.3‘4§+.~h- 3 " v” T?- 2712;; c1129.» m 3'5“: Me?" 4 ‘ «re/m" .1; '1')... pple , ...~..-.. 5 4‘: ‘ -- Jag/:5.-. 'r" \.v.- "If?“ ‘- v: .3;- 2‘28339 . 1' 7 m"“,‘n‘*dé§- -232; 8 7.52;; "“5: a; (_rfgo' EB L £118 "'3‘. 9 I .. .mé‘ :‘fi'f‘.’ 10 n. r: . .;~.:-_;x 'u.."- .. w i 9".) q f 1 1 skillet- "3 3. 12 ’1'?! ~.'.—.-'r~ v --- peaches , « - 13 salmon 7'" "‘ '— 14 m~n Emu) I, 351:": .3 .;:- - ' lo bowl ‘ _. 16 17 18 19 20 Correct (C): Correct (C): 93 LIST A: Short-Delay _Cued Recall Instructions: Tell me all of the shopping items from the Monday list that are: (category) Responses Spices 8.: Herbs: Fruits: Tools: Clothing: Correct (C): Perseverations (P): Intrusions (I): Time of day ShorbDelay Cued Recall completed: 94 LIST A: Long-Delay Free Recall Time of day Long-Delay Free Recall begun: Time of day Short-Delay Cued Recall completed: Toral delay: (Note: The total delay snould be about 20 minutes.) Instructions: I read some shopping items to you earlier: I'd likeyou to tell me all the items you can from the Monday list—that was the first list. the one that Iread to you five times. Go ahead. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 HHHI—IHHHHHH (DmQCDUItbOONO-‘O Sen-mule Cluster Correct (C): Perseverations (P): Intrusions (I): Semantic cluster score: 95 LIST A: Long-Delay Cued Recall Instructions: Ibll me all of the shopping items from the Monday list that are: (category) Clothing: 'Ibols: Fruits: Spices & Herbs: Correct (C): Perseverations (P): Intrusions (1): Summary Table of Recall Errors List A. g L f It: 1-5 ) Short-Delay Recall I Long-Delay Recall Error Type I Total List B l Free : Cued 5 Free g Cued Total ‘ l 1 I Perseverations l ' . 3 g l Intrusions: Free Recall : g Cued Recall 96 LIST A: Long-Delay Recognition Instructions: I'm going to read a list of shoppingitems. Afterl read each item. say “les"if the item was from the Monday list. and say “No" ifit was nor. (Note: For each “Yes" response. place a (w) in the one blank space to the right of the word: for each "No" response. leave the space blank.) A - List A-Correctu-lits) BS I 1.38: B-Shared BX . List B-Xonshared NP - NeztherList-Prot013pical PS - Neither ListPhonemicallySimilar L’N - Neither List-Unrelated Discriminability: 0 False Positives Misses ) x 100 (1- =(1-__)x100= Response Bias: False Post Live- Misses .. ______-___ TOTAL l I Mel’s-ins“ Mime BOSSIBLE l16 4 l 4 l [A BS Note: If False Positives = O or Misses = O. substitute a Total number of hits: value of 1 when computing - Response Bias. 'Ibtal number of false positives: Ulises = 16 - Hits = 97 APPENDIX E Wechsler Memory Scale — Logical Memory (Wechsler, 1955) Stgrv A I AM GOING TO READ TO YOU A LITTLE SELECTION OF ABOUT 4 OR 5 LINES. LISTEN CAREFULLY BECAUSE HHEN I AM THROUGH I WANT YOU TO TELL ME EVERYTHING I READ TO YOU. ARE YOU READY? (Do not re-read any portion of the story.) ANNA THOMPSON / or SOUTH / BOSTON, / EMPLOYED / L 4 As A SCRUH ROMAN / IN AN OFFICE BUILDING, / REPORTED / L 3 AT THE CITY HALL / STATION / THAT SHE HAD RHHN HELD UP / z 3 ON STATE STREET / THE NIGHT BEFORE / AND ROBBED / L 3 OF 15 DOLLARS. / SHE HAD FOUR / LITTLE CHILDREN, / L a Tag 3537 / HAS DUE, / AND THEY HAD NOT HATHN / z 3 FOR Two DAYS. / TH: ORRICHRS, / g 2 TOUCHHD BY THE RONAN'S STORY, / MADE up A PURSE / FOR HER. / l 3 TOTAL FOR STORY A (24 NOW HEAT DID I READ TO YOU? TELL ME EVERYTHING AND BEGIN AT THE BEGINNING. Record verbatim: 98 §torv B NOW I AM GOING TO READ YOU ANOTHER LITTLE SELECTION AND SEE HOW MUCH MORE YOU CAN REMEMBER ON THIS. LISTEN CAREFULLY. (Do not re-read any portion of the story.) THE AMERICAN / LINER / NEH YORK / STRUCH A NINE / z 4 NEAR LIVERPOOL / MONDAY / EVENING. / IN SPITE OF A BLINDING / 5 4 SNOHSTORN / AND DARKNESS, / THE SIXTY / ' 1 3 PASSENGERS, INCLUDING 18 / WOMEN, / HERE ALL RESCUED, / 1 3 THOUGH THE BOATS / HERE TOSSED ABOUT / LIKE CORHS / z 3 IN THE HEAVY SEA. / THEY WERE BROUGHT INTO PORT / 1 2 THE NEXT DAY / BY A BRITISH / STRANER. / 1 3 TOTAL FOR STORY B /22 NOW WHAT DID I READ TO YOU? TELL ME EVERYTHING AND BEGIN AT THE BEGINNING. Record RRERREIE= Scoring Story A: No. of Units Recalled = Story 5: No. of Units Recalled = Total No. of Units Recalled = 99 __ REFERENCES 100 I! REFERENCES . Albert, M. (1984). Assessment of cognitive function in the elderly. Psvchj_osomatics 25 310-317. American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author. Anschutz, L., Camp, C.J., Markley, R.P., & Kramer, J.J. (1985). Maintenance and generalization of mnemonics for grocery shopping by older adults. Experimental Aging Research, 11, 157-160. Baltes, PB. & Kliegl, R. (1992). Further testing of limits of cognitive plasticity: Negative age differences in a mnemonic skill are robust. Developmental Psycholpgy, 28, 121-125. Beck, AT. (1978). Beck Depression Inventogy. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation. Beck, AT. (1987). Beck Depression Inventory. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation. Beck, A.T., Steer, R.A., & Garbin, M. (1988). Psychometric properties of the Beck Depression Inventory: Twenty-five years of evaluation. Clinical Psychology Review, 8. 77-100. Beck, A.T., Ward, C.H., Mendelson, M., Mock, J., & Erbaugh, J. (1961). An inventory for measuring depression. Archives of General Psychiatm, 4, 53-63. Benson, H., Beary, J.F., & Carol, MP. (1974). The relaxation response. Psychiatgy, 37, 37-46. ' Brigham, M.C. & Pressley, M. (1988). Cognitive monitoring and strategy choice in younger and older adults. Psychology and Aging, 3, 249-257. Brown, W. A. (1988). Harnessing the placebo effect. Hospital Practice, 7, 1-14. Burke, D.M. & Light, LL. (1981). Memory and aging: The role of retrieval processes. Psychological Bulletin, 90, 513-546. Burt, D.B., Zembar, M.J., & Niederehe, G. (1995). Depression and memory impairment: A Meta-analysis of the association, its pattern, and specificity. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 285-305. 101 Cerella, J. (1985). Age-related decline in extrafoveal letter perception. Journal of Gerontology, 4Q 727-736. Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psycholpgical Bulletin 112 155-159. Crook, T., Bartus, R.T., Ferris, S.H., Whitehouse, R, Cohen, SD, & Gershon, s. (1986). Age-associated memory impairment: Proposed diagnostic criteria and measures of clinical change-Report of a National Institute of Mental Health work group. Developmental Neuropsychology, 2, 261-276. Craik, F.l.M. (1994). Memory changes in normal aging. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 3, 155-158. Craik, F.l.M. & Jennings, J.M. Human memory. In F.l.M. Craik & T.A. Salthouse, The handbook of aging and cognition. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Earlbaum Associates, Inc. Craik, F.l.M. & Lockhart, RS. (1972). Levels of processing: A framework for memory research. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 11, 671- 684. Craik, F.l.M. & McDowd, J.M. (1987). Age differences in recall and recognition. Journal of Experimenjtal Psychology: Lepming, Memorv. a_pc_l Cognition, 13, 474-479. Delis, D.C., Freeland, J., Kramer, J.H., & Kaplan, E. (1988). Integrating clinical assessment with cognitive neuroscience: Construct validation of the California Verbal Learning Test. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psycholpgy, gfi, 123-130. Delis, D.C., Kramer, J.H., Kaplan, E., & Ober, BA. (1987). The California Verbal Learning Test manual. San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich. Deptula, 0., Singh, R., & Pomara, N. (1993). Aging, emotional states, and memory. American Journal of Psychiatgy, 150, 429-434. Dikmen, S. S., Heaton, R. K., Grant, l., & Temkin, N. R. (1999). Test- retest reliability of expanded Halstead-Reitan Neuropsychological Test Battery. Journal of th_§ International Neuropsychological Sociey, 5, 346-356. Dobbs, A.R., Aubrey, J.B., & Rule, B.G. (1989). Age-associated release from proactive interference: A review. Canadian Psychology. 39_, 588-595. 102 Doolittle, N.O. & Wiggins, SD. (1993). Present and future health care for an aging society: A proactive self-help approach. Marriage & FJamily Review, 18. 57-71. Eysenck, MW. (1974). Age differences in incidental Ieaming. Development_al Psychology. 10. 936-941. Farkas, M.S. & Hoyer, W.J. (1980). Journal of Gerontology, 35, 207-216. Folstein, M.F., Folstein, S.E., & McHugh, PR (1975). Mini-Mental State. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12, 189-198. Gallagher, 0., Nies, G., & Thompson, L.W. (1983). Reliability of the Beck Depression Inventory with older adults. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 50, 152-153. Gerard, L., Zacks, R.T., Hasher, L., & Radvansky, GA (1991). Age deficits in retrieval: The fan effect. Journal of Gerontology: Psmholpgical Sciences 46 P131-136. Golomb, J., de Leon, M.J., Kluger, A., George, A.E., Tarshish, C., & Ferris, SH. (1993). Hippocampal atrophy in normal aging. Archives of Neurology, 50, 967-973. Gratzinger, P., Sheikh, J.l., Friedman, L., & Yesavage, J. (1990). Cognitive interventions to improve face-name recall: The role of personality trait differences. Developmental Psychology, 26, 889-893. Hartley, AA (1992). Attention. In F.l.M. Craik & T.A. Salthouse (Eds), The handbook of aging and cognition. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Earlbaum Associates, Inc. Hasher, L. & Zacks, RT. (1979). Automatic and effortful processes in memory. Journal of gyperimental Psychology: General, 108, 356-388. Hasher, L. & Zacks, RT. (1988). Working memory, comprehension, and aging: A review and a new view. In 6.6. Bower (Ed.), The psycholpgy of Ieaming and motivation (pp. 193-225). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Hutsch, D.F., Hertzog, C., & Dixon, RA. (1990). Ability correlates of memory performance in adulthood and aging. Psycholpgy and Aging, 5, 356- 368. Hultsch, D.F., Hertzog, C., Small, B.J., McDonald-Miszczak, L., & Dixon, RA. (1992). Short-term longitudinal change in cognitive performance in later life. Psychology and Aging, 7, 571-584. 103 Jennings, J.M. & Jacoby, LL. (1993). Automatic versus intentional uses of memory: Aging, attention, and control. Psychology and Aging, 8, 283-293. Jorrn, AF. (1986). Cognitive deficit in the depressed elderly: A review of some basic unresolved issues. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatm, 20, 1 1-22. Kane, M.J., Hasher, L., Stoltzfus, E.R., Zacks, R.T., & Connelly, S.L. (1994). Inhibitory attentional mechanisms and aging. Psychology and Aging, 9, 103-1 12. . Karel, M.J. (1997). Aging and depression: Vulnerability and stress across adulthood. Clinical Psychology Review, 17, 847-879. Kausler, DH. (1985). Episodic memory: Memorizing performance. In N. Chamess (Ed.), Aging and human performance (pp. 101-141). Chinchester: John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Kausler, DH. (1994). Learning and memory in normal aging. San Diego, CA: Academic Press, Inc. Kessler, R.C., Foster, 0., Webster, P.S., & House, JG. (1992). The relationship between age and depressive symptoms in two national surveys. Psycholggy and Aging, 7, 119-126. Kliegl, R., Smith, J., & Baltes, PB. (1989). Testing-the-limits and the study of adult age differences in cognitive plasticity of a mnemonic skill. Developmental Psychology, 25, 247-256. La Rue, A. (1992). Aging and neuropsycholpgical assessment. New York: Plenum Press. Larrabee, G.J. & Crook, TH. (1989). Dimensions of everyday memory in age-associated memory lmpainnent. Psychological Assessment: A Journal of Consulting and ClinLca;lPsychology,_1, 92-97. Light, LL. 8. Singh, A. (1987). Implicit and explicit memory in young and older adults. Journal of Experimental Psycholpgy: Learning, Memom, and Cpgnition, 13, 531-541. Madden, DJ. (1983). Aging and distraction by highly familiar stimuli during visual search. Developmental Psychology, 19. 499-507. 104 Madden, D.J. & Allen, PA (1991). Adult age differences in the rate of information extraction during visual search. Journal of G‘erontology: Psychological Sciences. 46, P124-126. McDowd, J.M. 8. Birren, J.E. (1990). Aging and attentional processes. Handbook of the psychology of agjpg (3rd ed., pp. 222-233), San Diego, CA: Academic Press, Inc. McDowd, J.M. & Filion, D.L. (1992). Aging, selective attention, and inhibitory processes: A psychophysiological approach. Psychology and Aging, 7, 65-71. Mirsky, AF. (1989). The neuropsychology of attention: Elements of a complex behavior. In E. Perecman (Ed.), Integrating theogy and practice in clinical neuropsycholpgy (pp.75-91). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Earlbaum Associates. Mitrushina, M. & Satz, P. (1989). Differential decline of specific memory components in normal aging. Brain Dysfunction, 2, 330-335. Mitrushina, M. & Satz, P. (1991). Reliability and validity of the Mini-Mental State. Journal of Clinical Psycholgqy. 4L 537-543. Mitrushina, M., Satz, P., Chervinsky, A., & D'Elia, L. (1991). Performance of four age groups of normal elderly on the Rey Auditory-Verbal Learning Test. Journal of Clinical Psychology. 47. 351-357. Morris, R.G., Craik, F.l.M., & Gick, ML. (1990). Age differences in working memory tasks: The role of secondary memory and the central executive system. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 42A, 67-86. Mungas, D., Ehlers, C.L., & Blunden, D. (1991). Age differences in recall and information processing in verbal and spatial Ieaming. Canadian Journal on Aging, 10, 320-332. Naveh-Benjamin, M. & Craik, F.l.M. (1995). Memory for context and its use in item memory: Comparisons of younger and older persons. Psychology and Aging, 10, 284-293. Neely, A.S. & Backman, L. (1995). Effects of multifactorial memory training in old age: Generalizability across tasks and individuals. Journal of Gerontology-Series B: Psychological Sciences & Social Sciences, SOB, P134- 140. Newmann, JP. (1989). Aging and depression. Psycholpgy and Aging, 4, 150-165. 105 Nissen, M.J. & Corkin, S. (1985). Effectiveness of attentional cueing in older and younger adults. Journal of Gerontolgqy, 40. 185-191. Pannalee, P.A., Katz, I.R., & Lawton, MP. (1989). Depression among institutionalized aged: Assessment and prevalence estimation. Journal of Gerontology, 44, 22-29. Plude, D.J. & Doussard-Roosevelt, J.A. (1989). Aging, selective attention, and feature integration. Psychology and Aging, 4, 98-105. Plude, D.J. & Hoyer, W.J. (1985). Attention and performance: Identifying and localizing age deficits. In N. Chamess (Ed.), Aging and human performance (pp. 47-99). Chinchester: John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Plude, D.J. & Hoyer, W.J. (1986). Age and the selectivity of visual information processing. flimal of Psychology and Aging, 1, 4-10. Prigatano, G. (1978). Wechsler Memory Scale: A selective review of the literature. Journal of Clinical Psycholpgy, 34, 816-832. Rapport, L. J., Axelrod, B. N., Theisen, M. E., Brines, D. B., Kalechstein, A. D., & Ricker, J. H. (1997). Relationship of IQ to verbal Ieaming and memory: Test and retest. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 19, 655- 666. Rockstroh, S., Dietrich, B., & Pokorny, R. (1995). Memory and attention test performance of young and elderly subjects after retest practice. International Psychpgeriatrics, 7, 377-384. Roy-Byme, P.P., Weingartner, H., Bierer, L.M., Thompson, K., Post, RM. (1986). Efforlful and automatic cognitive processes in depression. Archives of General Psychiatry. 43. 265-267. Ruegg, R.G., Zisook, S., & Swerdlow, NR (1988). Depression in the aged: An overview. Psychiatric Clinics of North Amerig, 11, 83-99. Russell, E. (1975). A multiple scoring method for the assessment of complex memory functions. Journal of Consulting anp_(_3linica_l Psycholpgy, 43, 800-809. Sack, S.A. & Rice, CE. (1974). Selectivity, resistance to distraction and shifting as three attentional factors. Psychological Reports, 34, 1003-1012. Salthouse, TA. (1988). Resource-reduction interpretations of cognitive aging. Developmental Review 8 238-272. 106 Salthouse, TA (1991). Theoretical perspectives on cognitive aging. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Earlbaum Associates, Publishers. Schaie, K.W. & Willis, S.L. (1993). Age difference patterns of psychometric intelligence in adulthood: Generalizability within and across ability domains. Psychology and Aging, 8, 44-55. Schmidt, M., Trueblood, W., Merwin, M., & Durham, R.L. (1994). How much do 'attention' tests tell us? Archives of Clinical Neuropsycholpgy. 9, 383- 394. Shum, D.H.K., McFarland, K., & Bain, JD. (1994). Assessment of attention: Relationship between psychological testing and information processing approaches. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsycholmy, E, 531-538. Shum, D.H.K., McFarland, K., & Bain, JD. (1990). Construct validity of eight tests of attention: Comparison of normal and closed-head injured samples. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 4, 151 -1 62. Sox, H. C., Margulies, I., & Sox, C. H. (1981). Psychologically mediated effects of diagnostic tests. Annals of lntemal Magicine. 95. 680. Spreen, O. & Strauss, E. (1991). A compendium of neuropsychological tests. New York: Oxford University Press, Inc. Stankov, L. (1988). Aging, attention, and intelligence. Psycholpgy and Aging, 3, 59-74. Storandt, M., Danziger, W.L., Berg, L., & Hughes, GP. (1984). Psychometric differentiation of mild senile dementia of the Alzheimer type. Archives of Neurology, 41, 497-499. Sweeney, J.A., Wetzler, S., Stokes, P., & Kocsis, J. (1989). Cognitive functioning in depression. Journal of Clinical Psycholpgy, 45, 836-842. Thomas, K. B. (1987). General practice consultations: Is there any point in being positive? British Medical Journal (Clinical Research Edition), 294, 1200. Titov, N. & Knight, RC. (1997). Adult age differences in controlled and automatic memory processing. Psychology and Aging, 12, 565-573. Tulving, E. (1972). Episodic and semantic memory. In E. Tulving & W. Donaldson (Eds.), Organization of memory (pp.381-403). New York: Academic. 107 Tulving, E. (1983). Elements of episodic memogy. Oxford, England: Clarendon. van Zomeren, A.H. & Brouwer, W.H. (1994). Clinical neuropsychologof attention. New York: Oxford University Press. Wechsler, D. (1955). Wechsler Memog Scale. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation. Weingartner, H., Cohen, R.M., Murphy, D.L., Martello, J., & Gerdt, C. (1981). Cognitive processes in depression. Archives of General Psychiatgy, _3_8_,42-47. West, R. & Bell, MA. (1997). Stroop color-word interference and electroencephalogram activation: Evidence for age-related decline of the anterior attention system. NeuropsychologL1L421427. Willis, S.L., Cornelius, S.W., Blow, F.C., & Baltes, PB. (1983). Training research in aging: Attentional processes. Journal of Educational Psychology, 75, 257-270. Yesavage, J.A. (1984). Relaxation and memory training in 39 elderly patients. Amprican Joprnal of Psychiaflv. 141, 778-781. Yesavage, J.A., Brink, T.L., Rose, T.L., Lum, 0., Huang, V., Adey, M., & Leirer, V0. (1983). Development and validation of a geriatric depression screening scale: A preliminary report. Journal of Psychometric Research, 17, 37-49. - Yesavage, J .A. & Jacob, R. (1984). Effects of relaxation and mnemonics on memory, attention and anxiety in the elderly. Experimental Aging Research, 19, 211-214. Yesavage, J.A. & Rose, TL. (1983). Concentration and mnemonic training in elderly subjects with memory complaints: A study of combined therapy and order effects. Psychiatm Research, 9, 157-167. Yesavage, J.A. & Rose, T.L. (1984a). The effects of a face-name mnemonic in young, middle-aged, and elderly adults. E_xperimental Aging Research, 10, 55-57. Yesavage, J.A. & Rose, T.L. (1984b). Semantic elaboration and the method of loci: A new trip for older learners. Experimental Aging Research, 10, 1 55-1 59. 108 Yesavage, J.A., Rose, T. L., & Bower, G.H. (1983). Interactive imagery and affective judgments improve face-name Ieaming in the elderly. Journal of @rontologv. 38, 197-203. 109 111041an 5797: UNIV. LIBRARIES llllllllllllllllllllllllllllllllllllllllWlllllHllllWllHll 31293021061647