fE‘K‘K‘JF'Ol Sf i a. r a. 5.2;”; , a : s .23 3% 5% C i) (7} LIBRARY Michigan State University This is to certify that the thesis entitled EVALUATING THE EFFECTIVENESS OF A SEXUAL ASSAULT PREVENTION PROGRAM TARGETING MALE COLLEGIATE ATHLETES presented by Wendi L. Siebold has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for M.A. degreein Psychology £4, Major professor Date M 0-7639 MSU is an Affirmative Action/Equal Opportunity Institution PLACE IN RETURN BOX to remove this checkout from your record. TO AVOID FINES return on or before date due. MAY BE RECALLED with earlier due date if requested. DUE DATE DUE DATE DUE 6/01 c:/ClRC/DaleDue.p65-p.15 EVALUATING THE EFFECTIVENESS OF A SEXUAL ASSAULT PREVENTION PROGRAM TARGETING MALE COLLEGIATE ATHLETES By Wendi Lyn Siebold A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF ARTS Department of Psychology 2000 ABSTRACT EVALUATING THE EFFECTIVENESS OF A SEXUAL ASSAULT PREVENTION PROGRAM TARGETING MALE COLLEGIATE ATHLETES By Wendi Lyn Siebold Male collegiate athletes are one of the groups on college campuses most commonly assumed to be at a high risk for perpetration of sexual assault, because of their direct involvement in a culture that has been known to foster an aggressive and competitive environment (Curry, 1991; Messner, 1992; Nelson, 1994). Yet, there exist only a handful of prevention and education programs that are specifically designed for male student athletes, none of which have been evaluated and published in the literature. The present study evaluated a sexual assault prevention program targeting male collegiate athletes. Initial results from this evaluation indicated that the program did not significantly reduce men’s rape myth acceptance or likelihood of sexually aggressing, or increase their likelihood of confronting. Additionally, none of the sport-specific characteristics of being an athlete significantly predicted participants’ change in attitudes post-intervention. Open-ended program utilization questions gave insight into these findings, as well as the possibility of group-level change within a male peer supportive environment (Schwartz & DeKeseredy, 1997). Implications for fiiture programming targeting male collegiate athletes are discussed, along with a general discussion about the efficacy of such efforts that target individuals who operate within a larger system of accountability. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS This study would not have been possible without the enduring support of numerous people who worked with me through both the process and the product of this project. I would particularly like to thank Jayne Schuiteman, the Women’s Resource Center, and all of the members of the program planning committee for allowing me to come on board and be a part of the this project from the start. I appreciate all of the support they gave to both me and the evaluation of this program — it was truly a joy to get to know and work with all of you. I am sincerely grateful to Cris Sullivan, the chair of my thesis committee, for advising me through the changing tides of evaluating such a politically tempestuous program. Her wealth of experience in program evaluation and working with campus groups helped guide me through the process. And if it weren’t for Cris, this paper would have probably been twice as long with multiple punctuation rnistakesl My other two committee members also deserve accolades, namely, Deborah Bybee for the amount of time she worked with me on my methodology and statistical analyses, and Pennie Foster-Fishman for guiding me through the “larger worth” conceptual portions of this project. Last, but certainly not least, I thank Carolyn for all of her emotional and practical support. She was my solid ground for not only the emotional aspects of this project, but also by being my own personal editor through reading numerous drafts of this paper. Thanks for putting up with all of my mood swings and supporting me throughout! iii TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES ........................................................................... INTRODUCTION ........................................................................... CHAPTER 1 OVERVIEW OF THE LITERATURE .................................................... Defining Sexual Assault ............................................................. The Etiology of Sexual Assault .................................................... Male Collegiate Athletics as a Rape-Prone Sub-Culture ........................ Rape Prevention Education .......................................................... F emale-Focused Programs ................................................. Mixed-Sex Programs ...................................................... Male-Focused Programs ................................................... Programs Targeting Collegiate Athletes ................................ Group-Level Change via Individual Change .................................... Conclusion to the Review ........................................................... Hypotheses ........................................................................... CHAPTER 2 METHOD ....................................................................................... Sample ................................................................................. The Program: Out of Bounds: An Anti- Violence Workshop for S tudent-A thletes at MS U ............................................................ Evaluation ............................................................................ Measurement .......................................................................... Statistical Analyses .................................................................. CHAPTER 3 RESULTS ..................................................................................... Pre-test Descriptives ................................................................ Multivariate Analyses ............................................................... Post-test Descriptives ............................................................... Predictors of Post-Intervention Change .......................................... Program Utilization .................................................................. CHAPTER 4 DISCUSSION ................................................................................. iv iv 1 6 6 10 22 28 29 31 34 37 40 44 46 47 47 47 S4 S6 73 75 78 84 88 93 100 104 LIST OF REFERENCES .................................................................... 131 APPENDICES ................................................................................ 140 Appendix A: Pre-Test Questionnaire ............................................. 141 Appendix B: Post-Test Questionnaire ........................................... 156 Appendix C: Sexual Assault Prevention Programs Targeting College Students .............................................................. 165 LIST OF TABLES Table 1 - Sport Orientation Subscale Item-to-Total Correlations ...................... 58 Table 2 - Individual Utilization Coding Categories ...................................... 69 Table 3 - Team Utilization Coding Categories ........................................... 70 Table 4 — Coding Categories for Factors Influencing Participants’ Confrontation of a Teammate ................................................... 72 Table 5 - Independent Sample T-Tests for Cases Excluded From Analyses Based on Selection Criteria .......................................... 76 Table 6 - Independent Sample T-Tests for Estimated Cases Based on Missing Partial Post-Test ...................................................... 77 Table 7 - Predictor Variable Scale Percentages at Pre-Test (measured on a 7-point scale ................................................................................... 79 Table 8 - Sport Orientation Scale Percentages at Pre-Test ........... . .................. 80 Table 9 - Percent Sexual Aggression Severity at Pre-Test .............................. 81 Table 10 — Percent Rape Myth Acceptance at Pre-Test ................................. 82 Table 11 - Percent Likelihood of Sexually Aggressing at Pre-Test .................... 82 Table 12 — Percent of Responses By Item for Likelihood of Confronting at Pre-Test ......................................................... 84 Table 13 - Correlation Matrix for Variables Used in Multivariate Analyses 85 Table 14 - Mean Scale Scores for Outcome Variables at Pre-Test and Post-Test ..................................................................... 88 Table 15 - Percent Rape Myth Acceptance at Pre and Post-Test ....................... 89 Table 16 - Percent Likelihood of Sexually Aggressing at Pre and Post-Test ......................................................................... 90 vi Table 17 - Percent Likelihood of Confronting at Pre and Post-Test .... ............... 93 Table 18 - Summary of Sequential Regression Analysis for Variables Predicting Participants’ Likelihood of Sexually Aggressing (Post-Test) ......................................................... 96 Table 19 - Summary of Sequential Regression Analysis for Variables Predicting Participants’ Likelihood to Confront (Post-Test) .............. 97 Table 20 - Summary of Sequential Regression Analysis for Variables Predicting Participants’ Rape Myth Acceptance (Post-Test) .............. 99 Table 21 - Factors Determining Individual Utilization .................................. 101 Table 22 - Factors Determining Team Utilization ....................................... 102 Table 23 - Factors Influencing Respondents’ Decision to Confront a Teammate ...................................................................... 103 Table 24 - Sexual Assault Prevention Programs Targeting College Students ........................................................................... 166 vii INTRODUCTION It is now known that sexual assault on college campuses is a major social problem. Recent studies suggest that undergraduate women are sexually victimized at three times the rate of women in the general United States population, and at least one in three undergraduate women experience an attempted or completed rape before leaving college (Boeringer, 1999; Koss, Gidycz, & Wisniewski, 1987; Meuhlenhard & Linton, 1987). Based on these findings, a number of colleges and universities across the country have implemented sexual assault prevention and education programs targeting the general student population. Although college students have been the recipients of a variety of prevention programs, specific sub-groups of the college population (e. g. fraternities and sports teams) have traditionally either missed general programming or have not received programming that is specifically tailored to their group’s reality. For example, sexual assault awareness programs are often targeted to first-year students on a voluntary basis, so students who elect to not attend these programs (who may ofien be men), or who are hard to access because of a busy schedule (e. g. athletes) do not receive any programming. However, recent theories about the etiology of sexual assault have increased the focus of speculation about the perpetration of sexual assault to certain “high—risk” groups of students (Sanday, 1990). Male student athletes are one of the sub-groups most commonly assumed to be at a high risk for perpetration of sexual assault, because of their direct involvement in a culture that has been known to foster an aggressive and competitive environment (Curry, 1991; Messner, 1992; Nelson, 1994). Factors that are both coached and expected within the world of sport, such as aggression and conquest over others, as well as the enforced belief that male power over women is natural and sexualized (Messner, 1992; Messner & Sabo, 1994), have led the general public, media, and some researchers to believe there is a connection between participation in all-male sports and the perpetration of sexual assault. Although there is a growing body of literature that makes connections between the sports culture and sexual assault, there is only a handful of prevention and education programs that work directly and specifically with male student athletes. Although these programs have reported having good outcomes and reputations with university administrators and students, none of the programs published in the literature has been evaluated for its effectiveness. The present study undertakes the evaluation of a college-based rape prevention program targeting male student athletes. The program is one of the few that targets males as a single-sex group in team-based workshops incorporating group facilitation and interactive presentation materials. It was developed in an effort to specifically address the unique culture that male collegiate athletes are a part of, and deliver the information in an effective way. The current evaluation adds not only to the general knowledge base of research on program content and presentation of information, but allows for a more critical look at what extraneous factors may predict the effectiveness of education programs that target male collegiate athletes. Considering the tailored nature of this program, the effectiveness of such an approach is important to understand to direct the efforts of fiiture programs targeting male collegiate athletes. It is necessary to not only determine if specific factors related to being a male collegiate athlete are related to program effectiveness, but also whether or not targeting male collegiate athletes for rape prevention education is even a worthwhile endeavor. The first section of this paper examines the definitional process that sexual assault has undergone over the past 30 years. There have been a variety of opinions about how sexual assault should be defined for use in research, and the specific definition that is used for this study, in addition to the development of presentation materials, is explained. In addition to there being a debate about how sexual assault should be defined, an even larger debate has surrounded the explanation for the perpetration of it. The second section gives an overview of the main theories of the etiology of sexual assault, and the underlying rationale for targeting male collegiate athletes with specifically designed rape prevention programs that differ from programs targeting the general male student body. The main theory reviewed is one put forth by Peggy Sanday (1990) in which America as a whole is deemed as a “rape culture,” or a culture that condones and facilitates the existence of rape. She and other researchers have hypothesized that certain groups within this rape culture (e. g. fraternities and male student athletes) are more prone to condone or commit sexual assault. A review of the research supporting the male collegiate athlete sub-culture as a rape culture is provided. Although there is evidence that the male collegiate athletic culture is conducive to increased sexual aggression against women, the factors connecting sports participation with sexual assault have been equivocal in the research. The third section of this paper reviews the recent research, theory, and debate on the specific connection between male collegiate athletes and sexual assault. The quantitative evidence of a variety of specific factors related to sexual aggression is presented, and suggestions of other possible factors are presented for firrther research. It is these sport-specific factors that may serve as predictors of a prevention program’s effectiveness. Therefore, these sport-specific factors were measured and tested as predictors of the current programs’ effectiveness at attitude change. Because the general link between student athletes and increased sexual aggression has provided evidence that athletes are indeed a higher risk population, there have been a few prevention and education programs developed that target male student athletes. However, in order to draw a distinction between these programs and programs that target the general student population, it was necessary to first gain an understanding of the rape prevention programs targeting the general college student population. There have been numerous programs developed targeting both male and female college students, and certain program formats and content have been found to be more effective with specific groups than with the general student body. For example, researchers have shown that, under certain conditions, men have less attitude change after a workshop than do women. As a result, there are programs targeting only men that incorporate specific presentation styles and formats to which men are more responsive. These findings within the general rape prevention literature help in the development of programs specifically targeting male collegiate athletes. Although specifically designed for male athletes, programs are based largely on techniques and materials used by programs targeting the general student body. It is therefore essential that the effectiveness of using general programmatic planning techniques for the implementation of a program specifically targeting male collegiate athletes is determined. The last section of the literature review describes the two main programs that have been developed and implemented specifically for male collegiate athletes. Although both of these programs report having great rapport with the school administration and athletes, neither one has been formally evaluated for effectiveness at attitude change or knowledge increase. Additionally, although one of the programs emphasizes the peer culture in which male student athletes exist, no evaluation of the effectiveness of this type of program focus has yet been evaluated. A discussion of the possibilities that exist for group change when individuals participate in programs like those that target collegiate athletes is then presented, as well as a description of the current program evaluation. Chapter 1 OVERVIEW OF THE LITERATURE Defining Sexual ASM Over the past 30 years, there have been a variety of opinions about how sexual assault should be defined for use in research. Much of the debate about the definition of sexual assault started and has centered on the largest and most widely cited survey of the prevalence of sexual assault on college campuses was administered by Koss and colleagues in 1987. In this landmark study, a self-report questionnaire was filled out by 6,159 students (3,187 women and 2,972 men) in 32 institutions of higher learning located across the United States. About 25% of the men in Koss’s original sample (Koss et al., 1987) reported engaging in some form of coercive sexual activity since the age of 14. Part of what has been most controversial about Koss et al.’s (1987) study is how sexual assault was measured. A range of coercive sexual behaviors were included in the scale, in order to reflect the dimensional view that sexual assault is a behavior that lies at one extreme of human sexual behavior. The two original goals for the creation of the scale were to 1) develop an instrument to collect data supporting the dimensional viewpoint and 2) develop an instrument capable of reflecting the large number of rapes that go unreported in the United States (Koss & Oros, 1982). Therefore, the scale captures a wide range of sexual behaviors that some people have considered unrelated to rape (Gilbert, 1991b; Sommers, 1994), while others consider them to be rape, whether or not the woman perceives it as so (e. g. women talked into having sex against their wishes). For example, Sommers (1994) claims that if a woman does not label an incident as rape, the researcher has no right to call her a rape victim, based on certain behaviors that took place in the situation. Despite this conceptual turf war, multiple people in the field of sexual assault research have maintained a dimensional view, which is still the most widely cited and used measure of rape prevalence in the US. (Bohmer & Parrot, 1993; Schwartz & DeKeseredy, 1997). More recent studies on the prevalence of college sexual assault have measured women’s and men’s experiences of a broad range of behaviors, rather than the classic legally-bound definitions of rape or criminal sexual conduct. Studies investigating the prevalence of not just rape, but all forms of sexual assault, including sexual coercion, have helped enlighten our understanding of the prevalence of sexual assault in America’s universities. With the use of a range of sexual behaviors to determine prevalence rates, many other researchers have found rates to range between 15% and 25% (Kanin, 1957; Koss & Gidycz, 1985; Koss et al., 1987; Makepeace, 1986; Muehlenhard & Linton, 1987; Rivera & Regoli, 1987). Expanding the concept of sexual assault has taken years. For years American society has perceived rape through stereotypic images (Lonsway, 1996). Rapists have been believed to be the outcasts of society — those who are often linked to crimes because of their income level, ethnicity, or mental health. We have been overwhelmed with news reports of serial rapists and deranged attackers — images that reinforce the belief that if women just avoid dark alleys and stay safe in their own homes and private spaces, rape will not affect them. This “stranger rape” ideology shaped American society’s understanding of sexual assault for many years, and only recently is beginning to change with more evidence that rape and sexual assault are much more commonly acted out among acquaintances. We now know that the majority of rapes (80-90%) are perpetrated by known acquaintances (Koss,1987; Lonsway, 1996; National Victim Center,1992; Warshaw,1988), and gradually, the general public as well as the media, medical, and legal professions are becoming more aware of the pervasiveness of acquaintance rape. However, broadening peOple’s understanding and definition of sexual assault to include behaviors such as sexual coercion and sex that is not penile-vaginal has been problematic. Many researchers and theorists still vary on their definition of sexual assault, with the main commonality being an emphasis on nonconsensual sexual conduct (Bohmer & Parrot, 1993; Schwartz & DeKeseredy, 1997). Feminist scholars and activists have argued for years that a broader definition of sexual assault that includes a range of unwanted sexual experiences is less judgmental and more encompassing of the violations that women endure throughout their lives, both emotionally and physically. For example, the use of alcohol and other drugs to facilitate a rape is increasingly common on college campuses (Koss & Gaines, 1993). Many women who have been drugged are not aware that what they experienced would be considered rape, largely because our society is slow to name unwanted sexual contact as “rape.” Often women expect sex to be somewhat “unpleasant” or “aggressive” with men, since American mainstream culture stresses heterosexual sex as being a man’s ultimate goal and source of pleasure, rather than something a woman should actively want or pursue (Brownmiller, 1975). This presents barriers to naming unwanted sex as rape, since these beliefs are so embedded in American culture. By capturing the totality of unwanted experiences that women endure, we gain a more representative picture of what college sexual life can be like, rather than excluding those equally devastating experiences that may not fit strictly legal definitions of rape or criminal sexual conduct (e.g. men’s use of alcohol or threats to gain sex). For the purposes of the current project, the definition of sexual assault that was developed by Koss et al. in 1987 and used by many other researchers since then (e. g. Schwartz & DeKeseredy, 1997), was used. This understanding breaks sexual assault into four categories: 1) Unwanted sexual contact — includes unwanted sex play (fondling, kissing, or petting) arising from menacing verbal pressure, misuse of authority, threats of harm, or actual physical force. 2) Sexual coercion — includes unwanted sexual intercourse arising from the use of menacing verbal pressure or the misuse of authority. 3) Attempted rape — includes attempted unwanted sexual intercourse arising from the use of or threats of force, or the use of drugs or alcohol. 4) Rape — includes unwanted sexual intercourse arising from the use of or threats of force and other unwanted sex acts (anal or oral intercourse or penetration by objects other than the penis) arising from the use of or threat of force, or the use of drugs or alcohol. The Etiology of Sexual Assa_ult_ In addition to the debates surrounding the definition of sexual assault, there have been a variety of theories about its etiology. Most theories of perpetration have focused on one or more of the following factors: 1) the individual, psychological make-up of perpetrators, 2) group dynamics, and 3) sociocultural phenomena. At this time, most researchers and theorists have come to an agreement that sexual assault is multidimensional in nature (Schwartz & DeKeseredy, 1997; Thorne-F inch, 1992; Malamuth, Stockloskie, Koss, & Tanaka, 1991; Craig, 1990; Baron & Strauss, 1989; Stanko, 1990). Individual, group, and sociocultural factors play a role in sexual and physical aggression by men against women. One example of the interrelatedness of these three factors is how group and sociocultural factors support the individual factors associated with sexual assault, by giving a person who is more prone to perpetration the environment and cultural support to actually carry out the assault. The individual component of a rape culture that is studied the most in relation to sexual assault is a person’s adherence to rape myths. Rape myths are “prejudicial, stereotyped, or false beliefs about rape, rape victims, and rapists” that are present in the general American culture, yet widely held by certain individuals (Burt, 1980, p. 217). Burt (1980) developed the first scale to measure an individual’s acceptance of rape myths, which included statements such as “A woman who goes to the home or apartment of a man on their first date implies that she is willing to have sex,” and “Many women have an unconscious wish to be raped, and may then unconsciously set up the situation in which they are likely to be attacked.” Lonsway & Fitzgerald (1994) have refined the definition of rape myths as “attitudes and beliefs that are 10 generally false but are widely and persistently held, and that serve to deny and justify male sexual aggression against women” (p.134). Rape myths are the most commonly measured variable in the evaluation of rape prevention programs, as an individual’s level of adherence to rape myths has been found to be associated with self-reported likelihood to sexually aggress (Koss & Gaines, 1993). Another aspect of an individual’s belief system associated with increased sexual aggression is an individual’s acceptance of the use of violence in both general society and interpersonal relationships. Burt (1980) also was the first to develop a scale that measured a person’s acceptance of interpersonal violence, and found that this construct was the strongest attitude predictor of rape myth acceptance. These findings have been replicated numerous times within the literature on rape prevention (Lonsway, 1996). Individual factors involved with rape perpetration, such as those described above, have been the main focus of empirical sexual assault research, as well as rape prevention interventions. However, there is a wide range of literature that focuses on how the group and cultural components of a society play a role in an individual’s choice of behaviors. It is these group/cultural components that have added to the speculation that male athletes are especially prone to sexually assault women, since they exist in a culture that is so dependent on peer-group support and teamwork. The understanding of the contribution of group or cultural factors to the occurrence of sexual assault was developed in the past fifteen years, mainly through qualitative research. One of the most prominent and widely cited cultural theories of rape perpetration was developed by anthropologist Peggy Sanday in 1990 in her landmark qualitative study of fraternities, Fraternity Gang Rape (Sanday, 1990). Sanday studied the existence of 11 rape in different cultural communities across the world, and based on those experiences abroad and her interviews and observations of fraternity members on American college campuses, she dubbed certain cultures “rape cultures.” Sanday’s claim that American society is a “rape culture” that supports the facilitation of rape is supported by previous writings about sexual assault. In her landmark book, Against Our Will, Susan Brownmiller (1975) asserted that rape is a “conscious process of intimidation by which all men keep all women in a state of fear” (p. 15). Martin and Hummer (1989) published a qualitative look at fraternity culture, and suggested that fraternities “foster and strengthen beliefs and attitudes supportive of sexual violence” (Boeringer, 1999, p.83). The theory of “rape culture” supports the contention that sexual assault is not just a result of one person’s biological make-up or psychological disposition; rather it is a behavior that is socially encouraged (Crosset, et al, 1995; Brownmiller, 1975; Russell, 1975; Sanday, 1981, 1990). Most definitions of a rape culture stem from Sanday’s (1981) study of ninety-five band and tribal societies, in which she classified 47% as rape-free and 18% as rape-prone (Sanday, 1996, p.193). Cultures that displayed a high level of tolerance for violence, male dominance, and sex segregation had the highest frequency of reported rape (both individual and gang) (Crosset, 1995). Sanday (1996) describes societies that are rape prone as those in which “the incidence of rape is reported by observers to be high, or rape is excused as a ceremonial expression of masculinity, or rape is an act by which men are allowed to punish or threaten women” (Sanday, 1996, p. 193). Schwartz and DeKeseredy (1997) have expanded Sanday’s (1996) concept by specifying the cultural components that make up a rape-prone culture. Among the multiple factors involved, there are a few that are more salient to the world of male sports 12 than others. First, the existence of a “courtship patriarchy” is an important component of the rape culture (Schwartz & DeKeseredy, 1997). Courtship patriarchies exist in societies that hold customs, beliefs, and opinions about the man’s role of dominance in a dating relationship. Expecting men to pay for dinner and a movie (the date), and expecting women to wait for men to open car doors are both examples of how the courtship process consists of traditional rules of behavior. Many people think these rules make life easy for women, because the man is “taking care” of her. However, these behaviors set up a dangerous power dynamic, as societies often are accustomed to engaging in behavioral “exchanges,” rather than a simple favor without anything expected in return. Schwartz & DeKeseredy (1997) comment that “the problem here as it relates to [sexual assault] is that people often feel that under the rules of what we will call ‘courtship patriarchy,’ the male under many circumstances is entitled to sex provided by the female” (p.62). Both men and women may at times feel pressured into ending a date with sexual intercourse or contact, although one or both parties may not want to engage in such behavior. Although the concept of a courtship patriarchy was originally developed in reference to general societal practices, there are some components of the male collegiate athlete experience that, when combined with a general culture supportive of courtship patriarchies, may exacerbate women’s vulnerability. Particularly, the loss of accountability and personal responsibility that often accompanies being a male student athlete (particularly on scholarship), may be a prominent factor involved with an athlete’s perception that he has the right to sex from a woman he dates or with whom he comes into intimate contact. Jeffrey Benedict, in his book Athletes and Acquaintance Rape l3 (1998) describes that in addition to multiple monetary and academic-related benefits, certain athletes also are given a type of privilege that may play the largest role in a player’s misuse of his power in an intimate situation: In addition to the isolation from typical student responsibilities, athletes are also relieved from virtually all personal accountability as well...As citizens, student athletes see their behavior condoned or overlooked when it violates either campus codes of conduct or local laws. Coaches and athletic administrators are often content to tolerate otherwise socially unacceptable behavior in exchange for superb athletic performances. (p.13) Other components defining a rape culture in our general American society may also be exacerbated through the world of male sports. Two of them stem from slightly different roots, yet result in a complementary oppression of women. The extent to which parents raise their children with traditional sex role beliefs and a mass media that sexualizes women as objects, combine to characterize the perceptions and expectations that men. and women have for their own role and the role of the opposite sex in intimate relationships and situations. A rape culture is often easily defined by its media images of women and their bodies in sexually demeaning and/or objectified roles. Examples of women in objectified positions often arise in all-male social gatherings, where fratemal bonding is at its highest point. Ice cube trays in the shape of nude women, glasses in the shape of a breast or with pictures of sexy women whose clothes disappear when the glass gets wet are all examples of objects turned into a representation of women’s bodies (Schwartz & DeKeseredy, 1997). Women are often reduced to just a part of their body — a breast, a nipple, armless, or headless. The pornographic industry and some advertisements for commercial products often depict women as sexual objects. In fact, pornography has been a primary target of feminists and anti-rape advocates for years. 14 They have argued that in heterosexual male-produced pornography, women exist to serve and service men, and that to some men “that means that women are fair game to rape or sexually abuse” (Schwartz & DeKeseredy, 1997, p.85). MacKinnon (1993) has made some of the most widely cited and publicized arguments against pornography, contending that pornography silences women and creates a hostile environment for women within general societal living (e. g. attaining jobs, housing, etc). Baker (1992) clearly describes the danger imposed to women specifically relating to sexual abuse: Pornography expresses more than the view that women are mere sex objects. It tells men that women enjoy sex and are always available for it, even when they deny it. It tells men that women secretly enjoy rape... (p. 140) While this view of women’s sexuality is often portrayed to the general American public as part fantasy, the availability of women for the sexual taking is often a reality for certain male student athletes. Highly recruited players are often “exposed to women who are used to entice players to their school [and] this initial experience with women as a benefit is expanded over the course of a college career” (Benedict, 1998, p. 14). Benedict (1998) notes that this practice is not carried out by college athletic departments, but rather by other players on the team. An NFL player interviewed by Benedict (1998), recalled the number of women (usually referred to as “groupies”) who were essentially at the sexual disposal of the team: There were groupies in college — college groupies. Athletic department college groupies are usually the ones that you meet on a recruiting trip. And they’re there and they sleep with all the recruits, or they sleep with some freshmen. It’s either someone that they [coaches and recruiters] are trying to entice to come to the school or someone that’s at the school and they want to keep happy. (p. 15) 15 Not all male student athletes have the “luxury” of available women and additional privileges, as “groupies” associate more with higher profile athletes and teams (e. g. basketball, football). However, other teams and athletes may assume that women who were known to be groupies are also sexual objects for the taking. When societal all-male group objectification of women is combined with the sexual privileges of certain male student athletes, it is possible that the extent to which men are trained to objectify women on the basis of sex creates a danger for those women who say “no.” When women are seen as objects and not as individuals, the boundaries get blurred between a woman who wants sex and a woman who wants companionship. This belief is reflected in our society’s practice of doubting the motives of women who claim higher profile athletes sexually abused them, particularly if the women are known to be “groupies” or have a promiscuous sexual history. Unfortunately, athletes’ experiences with groupies have not yet been empirically studied in relation to their likelihood of sexually aggressing, nor to the severity of their sexual aggression. A complementary cultural practice (that impacts individual beliefs) to the sexual objectification of women is the raising of children with extreme sex-role specific beliefs. For example, boys and girls who are raised to believe that maternal duties are for girls and being the family breadwinner is for boys often grow up with certain expectations for the other sex’s behavior in an intimate relationship (Thorne, 1993). Socialization theorists have written for years about the harm of growing up with such polarized expectations of gender, and some have tied this genderization to violence in intimate heterosexual relationships. Berkowitz et a1. (1994, p.7) argue that “men’s sexuality and their relationships with women provide a sphere for enactment and confirmation of these 16 traditional gender-role expectations, which assign men the role of aggressor and women the role of gatekeeper in sexual intimacy.” When the societal sexual objectification of women is concomitant with the engendered socialization of its girls and boys, individuals grow up into adults with belief systems supportive of a rape culture. It has been suggested that male collegiate athletes hold more stereotypic sex role beliefs for both themselves and the women in their lives (Parrot et al., 1994). Research has found that men who subscribe to traditional sex roles and male sexual dominance are more likely than other men to engage in verbal sexual coercion, sexual assault, and rape (Muehlenhard et al., 1990 as cited in Parrot et al., 1994). It is hard to argue with the connection of traditional sex role stereotypes and male athletes, since part of America’s definition of masculinity and “what it means to be a man” includes participation in sports. The implications for sexual assault are encountered in the fact that “masculinity in sport has been defined in contrast with, if not wholly separate from, women and womanhood” (Crosset et al., 1996, p.175). If a man hones his athletic talent enough to make a college team, he has “made it” into a world that defines its masculinity not only by stereotypical masculine behaviors (e.g. yelling, grabbing his crotch, being violent and unemotional unless the emotion is anger), but mainly to the extent it is not feminine. For example, it is common for coaches and teammates to call a player derogatory names when he fails to do something correctly in practice or a game. Some of the more common choices for names are “pussy,” “bitch,” and “girl” (Messner, 1992; Nelson, 1994). One of the accusations that boys in sports commonly grow up hearing is that they “play like a girl” or “throw like a girl” if their athleticism is not at par with the expectations of the activity. This polarization of gender is so extreme in male sports that even homosexuality between 17 men is engendered into being “feminine” and derogatory words about homosexual acts are often targeted at athletes. Nelson (1994), who has written extensively about the male sports culture, gives an example of how influential coaches can be in the process of defining athletes as non-feminine: Coaches who would be fired for calling athletes “nigger” employ with impunity such terms as “cunt.” “Faggot” is another popular derogation. It refers not so much to sex between men as to weakness, timidity, cowardice — and femininity. . .In response to “wimpy” performances, male coaches have been known to deposit tampons, sanitary napkins, and bras in young men’s lockers. “You have debased yourself to the level of a woman” is the message. (p.87) Cultural practices such as those described above, have led researchers to label certain sub-groups within the American rape culture as more “rape prone” because of their group characteristics often associated with sexual assault (e. g. male bonding, distinct hierarchies of power and dominance) (Sanday, 1996). The two most studied sub- groups are fraternities and male sports teams (Boeringer, 1999). Although the current study focuses on male collegiate athletes as a sub-culture, it is important to review some of the research regarding the fraternity sub-culture. Fraternities were the first sub-group to be studied, and likewise, most of the research on the dynamics of rape-prone sub- cultures comes from the fraternity literature. There are also some important connections and distinctions to be drawn between fraternity sub-groups and athlete sub-groups. Initial research with fraternities was more qualitative in nature, which helped describe and explain the type of culture that existed within fraternity houses. Sanday (1990) and Martin & Hummer (1989) have been the two main proponents (via ethnography) of there being a rape sub-culture within fraternities. Both provided convincing evidence that “fraternities may provide an environment in which rape- 18 supportive ideologies can thrive and be transmitted to others” (Boeringer, 1999, p.83). Social get-togethers in which fraternity men use alcohol, drugs, and physical and verbal coercion to “work a yes out,” as well as male bonding around the objectification of women and “scoring” sexually, all lend to the suggestion that fraternities are conducive to sexual assault (Boeringer, 1999). However, quantitative research that has tested the hypothesized connection between rape myth acceptance, sexual aggression, and affiliation with fraternities has not provided the convincing support that was expected. While many researchers have not been able to find a connection between fraternity affiliation and sexual aggression (Lenihan & Rawlins, 1994; Schwartz & Nogrady, 1996; Boeringer, Shehan, & Akers, 1991; Koss & Gaines, 1993), others have (Boeringer, 1999). Some have even found that fraternity members were less accepting of rape myths than a non-Greek comparison group (Schwartz & Nogrady, 1996). Both Schwartz & Nogrady (1996) and Koss & Gaines (1993) found that alcohol and substance use were stronger predictors of sexual aggression than fraternity affiliation. However, both had methodological problems, which may have had an impact on their findings. For example, Koss & Gaines (1993) warn against inferring too much from their study (1993), because of its low sample size of fraternity members. Schwartz & Nogrady (1996) caution that “it may well be that the large number of fraternity men who do not engage in sexually coercive behavior is masking the effect of those who do” (p. 159). Other studies have found that fraternity members were more likely to use alcohol, drugs, and other substances, even though they were less accepting of rape myths. Despite Koss & Gaines’ (1993) and Schwartz & Nogrady’s (1996) unsupportive findings, 19 Boeringer (1999) did find a relationship between fraternity and athletic affiliation and rape-supportive attitudes. Boeringer (1999) sampled general attitudes in a sample of 477 college men, and found that men who were members of fraternities scored significantly higher than the control group (of non-Greek and non-student-athlete men) on 18% of statements endorsing rape-supportive beliefs. The control group agreed with 8% of rape- supportive belief statements. Male collegiate athletes endorsed the most rape-supportive beliefs, agreeing with 56% of the statements (16 out of 28 statements). Boeringer’s (1999) study joins other quantitative and qualitative studies that have found similar evidence that fraternity groups hold more rape-supportive ideologies (Bohmer & Parrot, 1993; Schaeffer & Nelson, 1993; Sanday, 1990, Martin & Hummer, 1989). Because the research findings on fraternities and sexual aggression have been so inconsistent, some researchers and theorists have turned to a more detailed analysis of what is involved in the experience of a being fraternity member. Currently, peer group influence is one of the most prominent theories put forth, which suggests that an individual’s acceptance of rape myths or increased sexual aggression is largely influenced by a male peer group culture, in addition to other factors. Schwartz & Nogrady (1996) state quite simply that it may not be just fraternities, but rather the components that define a fraternity that make certain groups more rape-prone: It seems to be possible that other groups on campus may be just as likely as fraternities to provide extensive male peer support for the sexual objectification of women, and access to alcohol, that encourages some men to engage in victimizing behaviors. (p. 159) Schwartz & DeKeseredy (1997) are two of the main proponents of this idea and have written extensively about it in their book Sexual Assault On The College Campus: The Role of Male Peer Support (1997). They note that there are a variety of factors 20 associated with male peer group membership that have a relationship with being rape prone. For example, a group’s narrow conception of masculinity (e. g. quite accepting of the idea that males are dominant and females are submissive), group secrecy (e. g. hiding their peers’ deviant activities), and sexual objectification of women all are part of some male peer support groups. The concept of peer group influence particularly plays into the reasoning behind sports teams’ reputation as being the second most common group implicated in gang rapes (behind fraternities) (N eimark, 1991). Certain male collegiate athletes exist in a peer group culture that is quite similar to that of a fraternity group, including the male bonding that leads to the objectification of women and tight vows of secrecy that prevent exposure (Schwartz & DeKeseredy, 1997). As discussed earlier, certain male athletes are in an environment that continuously debases women and polarizes the sexes. However, the most salient part of the collegiate team athlete peer group culture is the daily training to sacrifice everything to a group goal and to exist “for the team.” Not only is each athlete a part of a team, he is part of a team that is trained to react without question to the orders and authoritarian directions of coaches and team captains. The hierarchy of team membership is set right from the start, as first year team members frequently go through “initiation rights” at the hands of older players (much like in fraternities), and the simple fact that most team captains are older, starting players. The male bonding that is essentially required on collegiate sports teams can be very powerful, since the athletes live, eat, and work with each other. Schwartz & DeKeseredy (1997) note that “this group bonding can be so strong that such men are willing to take part in rape, or to observe rape, or at least to take part in a cover-up, 21 because the alternative is to go against the group. It becomes more important to be part of the group than it is to do the right thing” (p. 126). This group influence also may account for the lack of research findings linking group members’ individual factors to sexual aggression. Boeringer (1999) alluded to the possibility that individual level factors (e.g. rape myth acceptance, acceptance of interpersonal violence) may not always have a direct relationship to sexual aggression, because such strong peer influence may essentially drown out individual decision-making and behavior in the guise of “being a team player.” Despite this, the relation that group factors have to sexual assault by members of the group has not yet been empirically explored. Because athletes are part of such a group-dominated culture (i.e. teams), it is necessary to explore the group influences involved with their level of sexual aggression or intervention in a sexually aggressive situation. More discussion about the effects of group influence will be discussed later in this chapter. Male Collegiate Athletics as_a Rape-Prone Sub-Culture There is little doubt among researchers and theorists that the male sports culture is indicative of a rape culture (Crosset et al., 1996). Male sports are highly sex segregated and often promote hostile attitudes toward rivals and physical domination over others (Kidd, 1990; Messner, 1992; Messner & Sabo, 1994). Organized male sports have been described as supporting male dominance and sexist practices (Crosset et al., 1996; Bryson, 1987; Kidd, 1990; MacKinnon, 1987; Messner, 1992; Whitson, 1990). Curry (1991) qualitatively assessed the language and culture within male locker rooms, and found statements consistent with what would exist within a rape culture. There are 22 perhaps two additional factors that are specific to the collegiate male athlete’s experience that are not accounted for in the peer group model presented above: lack of accountability and a struggle for autonomy. Issues of power, dominance, and privilege intertvvine in the daily life of a high profile collegiate athlete. On one hand, the athlete is often submissive to the orders of the coaching staff and older players on the team. Sport sociologists have noted that athletes exist in a life of “structured inequality” in which they are dependent on their coaches for playing time and exposure. This struggle for social placement has been suggested to be a contributing factor to male athletes’ sexual aggression against women off the field (Crosset, 1996). On the other hand, much of the general student body, some school administrators and staff, and numerous people affiliated with the university often treat athletes with more favor than non-athlete students, which serves as a constant reminder that the athlete is in a position of power and acquisition. It is possible that a athletes’ struggle for autonomy coupled with a heightened status on the college campus may play a role in some athletes’ violence against women. Research with college student athletes seems to be taking a similar path as research on fraternities. There is rich qualitative evidence that the male sports culture is a rape supportive environment (Messner, 1992, Curry, 1991; Nelson, 1994). However, there have been equivocal quantitative findings related to collegiate athletic involvement and sexual aggression. Despite some research supporting a connection between participation in collegiate sport and rape perpetration, the specific factors that contribute to sexual aggression remain undetermined (Koss & Cleveland, 1996). In fact, this lack of specific evidence has created quite a debate among researchers and the general public as to how or why student athletes may have a higher propensity to rape women. Some 23 claim that athletes are scrutinized more because of their notoriety, which creates a distorted perception that athletes are assaulting women at a higher rate than the general population (Dershowitz, 1994). However, members of the press have pointed out that athletic teams foster a sense of elitism, disrespect for women, and physical domination over others — which all can lead to a supportive environment for sexual aggression (Eskenazi, 1990). It turns out that both perspectives are probably accurate. As Crosset et al. (1996) stated, “The press has overstated the extent of the problem of athletes and violence against women, but evidence nonetheless suggests an association between athletic involvement and violence against women” (p. 164). Studies have found evidence to support both assertions (Crosset, Benedict, & McDonald, 1995; Koss & Gaines, 1993). In their study on the prediction of sexual aggression by alcohol use, athletic participation, and fraternity affiliation, Koss & Gaines (1993) found that the strongest predictor of self-reported sexual aggression in a sample of 530 male undergraduates, was drinking intensity, followed by nicotine use, hostility toward women, and athletic involvement. No effect was found for fraternity affiliation, which might be accounted for by the low sample size of fraternity members. For example, nicotine use contributed almost twice as much Beta weight power to the prediction of sexual aggression as athletic involvement. However, it must be noted that athletic involvement included a variety of levels of sports participation. Spectatorship, informal participation (e. g. pick-up games), club sports participation, and formal sports participation (i.e. varsity intercollegiate athletics) were all included in the variable athletic involvement. When this was broken down into specific components, formal 24 sports participation was more strongly associated with sexual aggression than other forms of athletic involvement. Crosset et al. (1995) followed up the work of Koss & Gaines by reviewing campus judicial records of sexual assaults on 30 campuses. Varsity athletes were found to be over-represented as reported perpetrators of simple sexual assault (one perpetrator as opposed to a group perpetration). For the combined three years that the judicial files encompassed, male student athletes accounted for 3.3% of the total male student population, but were 19% of the reported perpetrators. The authors note that caution must be taken when interpreting these results, as sample sizes were low (perpetrators n = 69), and the action of women’s reporting a sexual assault to campus police is not yet firlly understood. The notoriety of a student athlete may either encourage or discourage a woman’s decision to report the incident to campus police. The authors note that these findings “do not support the contention that athletes’ violence against women only appears to be a problem because athletes are being targeted by the media” (Crosset et al., 1996, p. 175). However, the media may still play a large role in the perceived notoriety of an athlete, which may or may not affect a woman’s reporting of the incident to campus pohce. The emphasis in current research has been to determine whether it is simply participation in varsity college sports that is related to violence against women, or whether violence is the result of behavior indirectly related to sport (e. g. hostile attitudes toward women, peer group support for violence) (Crosset et al., 1996). Part of the debate about whether or not athletes indeed have a higher rate of sexual assault is possibly due to the lack of specificity about what components of athletes’ experiences lead to sexual 25 assault. The direct relationship of sport participation to sexual aggression is blurred with additional factors that need investigation (Crosset et al., 1996). A variety of strategies have been suggested and used to distinguish between groups on campus that may have a higher or lower propensity to sexual assault. One suggestion has been to label athletic teams based on college student perceptions of them as “high-risk” or “low-risk”, with the recent finding that teams perceived as “high-risk” actually do seem to have a higher rate of sexual assault (Humphrey & Kahn, 2000). One of the strategies used to specify a particular component of the athletic peer group experience has been to group sports according to whether they are contact or non- contact. Contact sports, which involve players’ physical contact with one another, are sometimes assumed to be more fostering of aggressive behavior, and therefore, sexual assault (Caron et al., 1997). One study has shown that collegiate male athletes on competitive teams that also happen to be contact sports (e. g. football, hockey) are less egalitarian in their views toward women’s roles than their peers who are part of non- contact sports (e. g. track and field, swimming, golf) (Caron et al., 1985). However, a specific connection of participation in contact sports with sexual aggression or responsiveness to sexual assault prevention education has not yet been supported in the research (Koss & Cleveland, 1996). It has been suggested that it is not just whether a man participates in sports (or even contact sports), but that something else within the athlete’s experience might contribute to their increased likelihood to sexually aggress (Benedict, 1998; Koss & Cleveland, 1996). Researchers have reduced the variable of sports participation into various types of sport participation (e. g. contact/non-contact, club sports, varsity sports), 26 but very few have gone the step further to investigate what exactly about the athlete’s experience - not just the level of sport played — has a role in possible sexual aggression. Perhaps it is not just simply the type of sports participation, but rather a combination of specific athlete characteristics or behaviors associated with sport participation and the team “type” that are associated with increased sexual aggression toward women. The only study to date that has looked at additional sport-specific individual factors is Caron et a1 (1997). Using the Sport Orientation Questionnaire (Gill & Deeter, 1988), they measured not just type of sport participation, but also level of competitiveness, win orientation, and goal orientation. Their study is the most recent to distinguish between level of sports involvement (high school v. college), and compare level of involvement to specific athlete characteristics (e. g. competitiveness). A significant positive correlation was found between athletic participation in high school and competitiveness, but no significant correlations were found between athletic participation and win orientation or goal orientation. However, the relationships between sport participation in college and scores on all three of the Sport Orientation Questionnaire’s subscales were positively significant, indicating that intercollegiate athletes have more extreme levels of sport-specific personality characteristics (6. g. competitiveness). It is important to view these results in consideration of how such athlete-specific traits may play into abusive behaviors toward women (e. g. sexual assault). Many athletes are taught to use force to settle a conflict or problem (Caron et al., 1997), and competitiveness is necessary to win over an opponent. Winning is known to be the most important part of competition for most athletes (Kang et al., 1990; Toufexis, 1990) and 27 athletes learn to use their bodies as a “tool, machine, or even a weapon” in order to “defeat the objectified opponent” (Messner, 1987, p.59). When men see women as an opponent, or see sex as an accomplishment or competition, such views at most may seriously increase a man’s ability to behave in sexually assaultive ways toward women. At least, such views may impede a man’s likelihood of understanding issues related to sexual assault against women, especially if such aggressive behaviors are considered normative in the athletic culture in which he exists. Although the above findings are helpfirl in determining the intra-individual factors involved in an athlete’s sexual aggression, there has been no empirical research on the group factors that may play a role in an athlete’s decision to sexually aggress or prevent a teammate from sexually aggressing. Although most of the qualitative accounts of the male athlete culture describe the strength with which the team holds together and enforces uniform behavior among its members, empirical studies have only measured intra-individual characteristics of each player (Crosset et al., 1996). It would be helpfiil to measure to what extent the team component of an athlete’s collegiate sport experience has on his willingness to take part in sexually aggressive activities or to intervene with teammates who are doing something sexual to a woman who is not willingly participating. Rape Prevention Education Although researchers have not identified the specific causal link between collegiate male sport culture and sexual aggression, enough evidence of a prevalence of sexual assault by athletes has been found to support the need for prevention education among student 28 athletes (Crosset et al., 1996; Crosset et al., 1995; Koss & Cleveland, 1996; Koss & Gaines, 1993). There are a few rape and dating violence prevention programs in the country that have targeted college student athletes (Parrot, 1994; Katz, 1995). These programs will be discussed in more detail in later sections, as first a discussion of the rationale and findings related to sexual assault prevention education is necessary. Although most prevention education programs have not focused on collegiate athletes, there are findings within the field that can be used in the design of effective programs with collegiate athletes. Over the past twenty-five years there has been an increasing number of studies and theoretical papers focusing on rape and its prevention. A variety of disciplines, including social work, psychology, criminology, and sociology have looked at the issue of rape prevention through different lenses of analysis. Traditionally, researchers and theorists have focused the prevention of sexual assault on either individual “risk-factors” (a man’s poor impulse control, or a woman’s ‘provocative’ attire), or specific situational factors (drinking and dark alleys). A sample of prevention program strategies has included: increased campus lighting, self-defense training, educational presentations for high school and college students, media campaigns, and role-playing. In their 1987 review of rape prevention strategies, Fischoff, Furby, and Morgan found a total of 1,140 possible strategies. Femle-focused prgggms. In the last ten years, in response to the findings that college women are more at risk for being sexually assaulted than any other age group (Koss, 1987), there has been an increase in the number of programs that target female college students. Many colleges have attempted to educate women about the risks 29 associated with sexual assault and “protective measures” they can take to increase their safety. Unfortunately, most colleges lack programs aimed at men and their own role in campus sexual assault. Despite some philosophical support of programs targeting females (Breitenbecher & Scarce, 1999; Hanson & Gidycz, 1993; Yeater & O’Donohue, 1999) there has been a sea of debate surrounding the ethical underpinnings of these programs as well as doubt as to their ultimate effectiveness in preventing rape. A wide number of researchers and theorists have been involved in the discourse around the efficacy of female-focused interventions (Katz, 1995; Funk, 1993; Berkowitz,1994; Lee,l987; Schmidt & Peter, 1996; Matabuti,1993; Lonsway et al.,1998; Foubert & Marriot,1996; Ring & Kilmartin, 1992; Schewe & O’Donohue,1993 and 1996; Dworkin,1993). These writers have all contended that when we limit intervention programs to women and girls, and teach them to behave in “rape appropriate” ways, we are sending the message that they can prevent a rape, and that they themselves are in control of whether they are raped or not. As F eltey et al. (1991) put it, “When only females are educated and informed, they are ultimately being held responsible for their own victimization.” Lonsway (1996) has called such “prevention” efforts more of a deterrent approach, rather than actual prevention: Because men who rape select potential victims on the basis of vulnerability (Brownmiller, 1975), it makes sense that a deterred attempt will only result in the victimization of another, more vulnerable individual. Rape deterrence strategies can therefore only protect individual women (albeit with no guarantees), but can never reduce the vulnerability of women as a group (p.232) Schewe & O’Donohue (1996) reason that: Not only are these forms of “prevention” unacceptable on the basis of a severe restriction of basic human rights, but also these tactics 30 will never be completely successful. Despite the fact that women take these and other precautions, the fact remains that no one can be constantly and perfectly vigilant. Thus, no matter how well trained potential victims become in avoidance, escape, and physical self-defense, they will be vulnerable to rape to the extent that there are men who will attempt to commit acts of sexual violence (p.456) While such programs give women the responsibility for stopping rape, they also ignore the fact that violence against women concerns men as well as women. Katz (1995), who has deve10ped an extensive violence prevention program for male collegiate athletes, considers this lack of male inclusion one of the main factors involved with violence against women. He states, “Calling this violence a “women’s issue”. . .is in fact part of the problem. Why? It sends a signal to guys that it is not our concern: Why would a man concern himself with women’s issues?” (p.163) Mixed-sex programs. Katz, as well as a good number of other researchers and activists (Berkowitz,1994; Dworkin,1984; Foubert & Marriot,1996; Funk, 1993; Lee, 1987; Lonsway,1998; Matabuti,1993; Ring & Kilmartin,1992; Schewe & O’Donohue, 1993 and 1996; Schmidt & Peter,l996) has started approaching the issue of sexual violence prevention from a male inclusionary perspective. These researchers have acknowledged the fact that when a woman is raped it affects men as individuals and as a whole, and there is sufficient reason for men to become involved in the movement to end rape. As an effort to include males in the prevention of rape, mixed-sex programs as well as male—focused programs have been developed and some evaluated over the past fifteen years. Operating on the assumption that both males and females will benefit from a change in rape-supportive attitudes, many programs have been set up that target a mixed- 3l sex audience, usually in a classroom setting. This notion of mutual benefit has been a topic of theoretical discussion within the literature for years (Berkowitz, 1994), and has more recently been applied to the study of rape prevention education. A common format for mixed-sex interventions has been a lecture-based presentation with or without the use of brochures and efforts to reduce rape myth adherence. One of the most widely used techniques in rape prevention programs is the challenging of adherence to rape myths, as well as a measure of a person’s acceptance of interpersonal violence and hostility toward women. Most mixed-sex programs that entail some form of dispelling rape myths report positive short-term outcomes among women and occasionally some men. However, men in general are more resistant to rape prevention programs, adhere more strongly to rape myths than do women, and support sexually coercive behavior (Buckley & Masters, 1992; Feltey, Ainslie, & Geib, 1991; Heppner, Humphrey, Hillenbrand-Gunn & DeBord, 1995; Lenihan, Rawlins, Eberly, 1992; Proto-Campise et al., 1998). So while women leave the program with a significant amount of positive attitude change, men do not always have the same magnitude of attitude change, and may even become more resistant to attitude change (Berg, 1993; Borden, Karr, & Caldwell-Colbert,1988; Krulewitz & Kahn,1983; Lenihan et al., 1992; Proto-Campise, 1998) . In addition, it has been shown through follow-up surveys that men have a higher “rebounding” rate, or return to original ideals than women after an intervention (Heppner et al., 1995). This has consistently been found in the literature for over thirty years (Borden et al., 1988; Lonsway, 1996). One way of teasing apart the gender difi’erences has been to differentiate between rape-specific attitudes and more general, contextual level variables, such as sex role 32 stereotyping (F onow et al., 1992). Rape specific attitudes have mainly been measured by the rape myth acceptance scale developed by Burt in 1980, or a variation of the scale into some related form. Contextual-level variables have included measurement of attitudes on a variety of social and interpersonal beliefs (e. g. sex role stereotyping, hostility toward women, and acceptance of interpersonal violence). It has been widely supported in the literature that adherence to contextual-level variables is strongly related to adherence to rape myths and more rape supportive attitudes (Fonow et al., 1992). However, the majority of evaluated mixed-sex programs that report some reduction of rape-supportive attitudes also have found little or no significant change in contextual-level attitudes for both males as well as females (Dallager & Rosen, 1993; F onow et al., 1992; Lonsway, 1996). Because contextual-level variables make up the day-to-day worldview of an individual, it has been suggested that only programs that include an in-depth discussion of societal norms and stereotypes can accomplish this level of attitude change (F onow et al., 1992; Lonsway, 1996). The gender gap that exists in males’ and females’ understanding and acceptance of rape-supportive attitudes has been well documented (Proto-Campise,1998), and researchers are now beginning to acknowledge that rape prevention education needs to take into account the specific needs of males and females (F onow et al., 1992). However, this shift has been slow in coming. The majority of programs still have outcomes that reflect this gender difference in attitudes, and researchers have continuously acknowledged that it is a problem, yet have rarely gone beyond a mention to create a conducive solution. 33 _M_ale-focused programs. A number of strategies have been implemented to tap into the complex attitude changes required of males. The recent emergence of all-male (or male-focused) programs has brought some insight into more effective strategies for attitude change with males. As a whole, male-focused programs are more intense and interactive than mixed-sex or all-female programs, with the intent of helping men recognize and understand the implications rape has on themselves and the loved ones in their lives. Researchers are now starting to recommend the use of all-male programs mainly because their interactive and intense atmosphere affords the focus and attention on male-specific culture and constructs (sex roles, masculinization) that may promote significant behavioral change (Yeater & O’Donohue, 1999). One factor that seems to be stressed more than others in male—focused programs is generation of empathy for a victim of sexual assault. Lonsway (1996) notes that “because empathy leads to more situational attributions for the behavior of others, it is reasonable to suggest that individuals with more empathy for victims will be less likely to attribute rape to personal characteristics such as victim appearance or behavior, and more likely to blame the man or society for the attack” (p.249). Empathizing with the victim and women’s constant threat of rape also helps men understand that any woman in their lives may have been or may be in the fiiture a victim of rape. In her review of acquaintance rape education programs, Lonsway (1996) found that three programs for men have reported desirable attitude change with a strategy of empathy induction (Gilbert et al, 1991; Gray et al, 1990; Lee, 1987), whereas another two have not (Berg, 1993; Ellis et a1, 1992). The reason for this difference is not very clear, since both sets of programs used similar techniques for inducing empathy. All of these 34 programs used empathy inducing techniques that included having the group of male participants either listen to, read about, or watch a video of another man or boy tell his story and experience of being a victim of sexual assault. This strategy of “seeing oneself as a potential victim” was used in these programs to induce empathy fiom the participants, and conclusively lead to higher sensitivity to issues around rape and less rape myth acceptance. However, only the first three programs (Gilbert et al, 1991; Gray et al, 1990; and Lee, 1987) reported a positive outcome and increase in empathy for rape victims, whereas the other two programs (Berg, 1993; Ellis et a1, 1992) reported no increase in empathy. In fact, Berg et al (1993) had divided the intervention into two groups of men: one that heard a woman’s story of being assaulted, and another that heard a man’s story of being assaulted. Both groups reported no significant change in victim empathy, while the group that heard the woman’s story actually reported a greater likelihood of sexual aggression than before hearing the story. Berg (1993) and Lonsway (1996) have both hypothesized as to why men may have trouble at first with victim empathy. One idea is that since acquaintance rape is largely seen as a crime in which women are the victims, men automatically assume the role of observer or aggressor (Berg, 1993). Programs that are more intense and involve personal interaction may allow men the freedom to work out feelings of anxiety and resistance to prevention programs in general. This need for increased intensity in education programs involving men is something that has been voiced by many researchers. Borden et al. (1988), after unsuccessful efforts at changing attitudes through lecture presentations, argued for “new, more dynamic, vivid interactive program formats to enhance the desired efl’ects of 35 consciousness raising, attitude change, and empathy toward rape” (p. 135). This suggestion, coupled with the finding that men do not easily empathize with rape, may account for the fact that there is so much resistance and lack of attitude change among men. A common strategy for enhancing intensity is interactive participation by the participants. Programs that include some form of participant interaction generally report outcomes with desirable attitude change (Lonsway, 1996). Interactive program techniques may entail small discussion groups, role-playing (Gilbert et al., 1991; Gray et al., 1990; Ring & Kilmartin, 1992), and even interactive dramatic performances (Mann et al, 1988). The importance of such interactive participation in programs that involve changing men’s attitudes, is just now being fully understood. For example, a program evaluated by Gilbert, Heesacker, & Gannon (1991) presented a combination of lecture- based information about rape as well as empathy induction techniques to 75 undergraduate men. Male participants reported feeling more comfortable with the presented lecture material only after partaking in the discussion group. Lonsway (1996) notes that “such interaction apparently played an important role in addressing men’s concerns, thereby easing their discomfort” (p.248). This “discomfort” may be part of the resistance seen from men to empathy-inducing strategies, as well as prevention programs in general. Since most interventions are short-term and are held in a mixed-sex classroom setting, men may be dealing with a wide range of emotions that are brought up by the setting of the program itself. Theorists like Funk (1993) have noted how intense feelings of guilt often encompass men when the subject of rape is addressed. This guilt is uncomfortable, and to avoid this discomfort, men often choose to resist what messages 36 are causing their feelings of guilt. Discussion groups and more interaction among audience members may help to override men’s initial guilt that provokes resistance to sexual assault prevention programs. Another benefit of all-male groups is that in some ways they replicate the all-male groups in which rape-prone male bonding occurs. Especially in relation to student athletes, who exist as a unified group (i.e. team) through most of their years in college, this all-male group structure is a place to more appropriately address many of the activities than male peer groups engage in and feelings that men may have that are conducive to a rape prone sub-culture. Yet, only one program targeting male collegiate athletes has discussed sexual violence against women in the context of peer group influence, and this program has not yet evaluated its effectiveness at attitude or behavior change (Katz, 1995). groggms targeting collegiate athletes. There are a few rape and dating violence prevention programs in the country that work with male collegiate athletes. A few have been described in the literature, yet none of those published have been formally evaluated. One prevention and education program was designed for and used with student athletes at Cornell University in 1990. Both the men’s rowing team and football team participated in the workshop, the goals of which were to “educate students about the risks and consequences of committing sexual assault, the behaviors that can lead to rape, and the consequences of these acts” (Parrot et al., 1994, p. 180). The program also attempted to “challenge attitudes that demean or objectify women and the messages and norms that condone and perpetuate a rape-prone environment” (Parrot et al., 1994, p.180). 37 According to the authors, this program was well received by both the players and the athletic department, and was given firnding to continue in firture years with certain teams. Although the authors were able to suggest certain steps to take in the development and implementation of a program with collegiate athletes, actual attitude or knowledge change among the athletes was not formally evaluated. In addition, the narrow scope of the program (only rowing and football took part in the program) disallowed any exploration of sport-specific factors that may play a role in the effectiveness of such a program (e. g. contact or non-contact sport, team influence). Katz and his colleagues (1995) have designed and implemented perhaps the most extensive sexual assault education program with male student athletes. The Mentors in Violence Prevention Project (MVP) trains student athletes (both male and female) to work on campus to prevent rape, battering, sexual harassment, and other forms of violence against women (Northeastern University Student Handbook, 1999). The MVP program is focused on the underlying assumption that the “sociocultural construction of manhood is central to the problem of men’s violence against women, as well as the basis of potential sources of prevention” (Katz, 1995, p.163). For the men’s program, two male facilitators go into the locker rooms of men’s college teams and do a series of workshops with each team. They work in an all-male environment and incorporate exercises that induce empathy, increase interaction among the team members, and directly address issues of masculinity, male peer groups, and athletes’ celebrity role and power on the college campus. One of the more innovative characteristics of the MVP program is its focus on the influence that the team atmosphere has on collegiate athletes’ decision-making. Specifically, MVP directly discusses the 38 “bystander effect” that occurs when one or more teammates socially misbehave in group settings (e. g. a party) and other teammates, although not directly participating in the behavior (e. g. raping the woman), stand to the side and watch, without intervening to stop the other teammates’ actions. Although many of the men who are bystanders may be uncomfortable with the behaviors of their teammates, the social pressure to conform to the group (i.e. team), and not deviate from the group norm is especially strong in a team environment that’s purpose is to achieve the goal of athletic success (Hackman, 1976). Berkowitz (1994) has found evidence that many men feel uncomfortable when other men brag about their sexual exploits or comment on women’s bodies in sexually suggestive ways. These men have been labeled as belonging to a “silent majority” of men who “keep their discomfort to themselves rather than express disagreement or intervene in an environment which they perceive as unsympathetic” or as threatening their status in the group itself. Although not directly participating in the behavior, lack of intervention is a serious problem that allows the behavior to continue without question. The MVP Program talks with men about their bystander role in such situations, and helps them work on ways in which they can intervene, as well as the consequences of such intervention. Although Katz et al. (1995) report “success” through the acceptance of their program and the use of the Playbook, a manual of activities used in the program, they have not yet published a formal evaluation of attitude or knowledge change. Even more striking is the fact that the MVP program is the only published program that directly targets the peer-group component of the male collegiate athlete culture, yet this component, to date, has gone without evaluation. 39 Group—Level Qhange viflndividual Chflge Considering how much peer support is involved in the male athletic culture, it is necessary to be familiar with and use the various theories pertaining to group-level change via the individual person. The following section reviews some of the work within the social psychological field of minority dissent and how this may affect group norms of behavior. According to this literature, there are two ways that a training such as the one evaluated in the current study can help create group-level change: 1) programs targeting collegiate athletes can help individuals to withstand group pressure and be minority dissenters, and 2) the training can help minority dissenters feel confident enough to stand up to group pressure and gradually change group norms of behavior. Both components will be discussed in the following section, starting with a discussion about the group dynamics through which norms and social pressure occur on a team. Understanding these dynamics will help clarify how a group can be influenced by one or more individuals within that group, which will be discussed later in this section. Group pressure and norms of behavior. Athletic teams are especially prone to intense levels of group (i.e. peer) pressure, mainly because of their structural make-up and purpose. The main purpose of a team is to attain the goal of being successfirl against opponents in the game of sport. As Hackman (1976) notes, one of the conditions for which a group often exerts more social pressure on its members is to produce uniformity for the purpose of attaining a common goal. Hackman (1976) explains, “it may be usefirl for group members to hold similar beliefs about the external environment, especially if the group must respond as a unit to that environment” (pg. 1473). Collegiate sports 40 Group—Level Change via Individual Change Considering how much peer support is involved in the male athletic culture, it is necessary to be familiar with and use the various theories pertaining to group-level change Via the individual person. The following section reviews some of the work within the social psychological field of minority dissent and how this may affect group norms of behavior. According to this literature, there are two ways that a training such as the one evaluated in the current study can help create group-level change: 1) programs targeting collegiate athletes can help individuals to withstand group pressure and be minority dissenters, and 2) the training can help minority dissenters feel confident enough to stand up to group pressure and gradually change group norms of behavior. Both components will be discussed in the following section, starting with a discussion about the group dynamics through which norms and social pressure occur on a team. Understanding these dynamics will help clarify how a group can be influenced by one or more individuals within that group, which will be discussed later in this section. Group pressure and norms of behavior. Athletic teams are especially prone to intense levels of group (i.e. peer) pressure, mainly because of their structural make-up and purpose. The main purpose of a team is to attain the goal of being successfiil against opponents in the game of sport. As Hackman (1976) notes, one of the conditions for which a group often exerts more social pressure on its members is to produce uniformity for the purpose of attaining a common goal. Hackman (1976) explains, “it may be usefirl for group members to hold similar beliefs about the external environment, especially if the group must respond as a unit to that environment” (pg. 1473). Collegiate sports 40 teams eat meals, travel, room, and socialize with each other throughout the calendar year, essentially sharing both the sports world, as well as their external social environment. Outside of their task of playing a sport, teams often have a vested interest in keeping one another uniform and acting as a whole to preserve the integrity of the team. Hackman (1976, pg. 1474) again explains that “groups may ... seek uniformity for purely “maintenance” reasons; that is, keeping the group intact and functioning as a unit, independent of task-related activities. Too much individualistic or idiosyncratic behavior on the part of a few members, for example, can threaten the very survival of the group.” One of the ways that groups attain and maintain uniformity is by establishing group norms of behavior and attitudes, with which members of the group are expected to follow, or risk exclusion from the group (Hackman, 1976; Moskovici, 1980). Changing group norms: the role of the minority dissenting opinion. Despite the lack of independence among group members for fear of group exclusion (Hackman, 1976; Moskovici, 1980), it is possible for a member of a group to dissent from the majority opinion and change group norms. This person takes the role of a rrrinority dissenter, or one who holds a minority dissenting opinion. One of the main factors that determines whether or not a member of a group will comply with group norms is whether or not the behavior expected by the norm is congruent with the personal attitudes and beliefs of the individual group members (Hackman, 1976). This is perhaps the component of group norms relating to behavior change that is most relevant to intervention with male student athletes. It is possible that by changing or enforcing a team member’s attitudes or beliefs about sexual assault issues, that he would have the tools needed to deviate from the group norm. In doing so, he would be one step closer to 41 providing the rrrinority dissenting opinion that is the first step to group-level change (Nemeth et al., 1988). It has been shown that exposing a group that holds a majority viewpoint to a person who is willing to offer a dissenting opinion is a very powerful mechanism by which to increase the chances that other group members will develop independent judgments, rather than relying on group norms (Nemeth et al., 1988). Although people sometimes assume minority opinions are incorrect, and therefore dismissed, perseverance and consistency by the minority person increases the chances that the majority group takes the minority viewpoint seriously and considers alternate opinions (e. g. “How can that person be wrong, and yet so sure of themselves?”). The majority group is “stimulated to reappraise the entire situation, which involves a consideration of numerous alternatives, one of which is the position proposed by the majority” (N emeth, 1986, p. 26). Essentially, it is not whether or not the minority opinion is correct, but rather simply that he or she spoke up against the majority view, resulting in the majority opinion being opened to questioning from all sides. As Nemeth (1986, p. 26), explains, “as such, the thought processes [of the majority group] are marked by divergence and hence, the potential for detecting novel solutions or decisions.” This divergence within the group is what contributes to individual group members’ level of independence and ability to withdraw from group normative behavior (N emeth et al., 1988). Individuals are faced with confrontation and a divergent viewpoint, which “makes each individual a better problem solver or decision maker by stimulating him or her to examine and reexamine premises” (Nemeth, 1986 p. 30). Nemeth (1986, p. 30) again explains that “the presence of dissenting minority views and 42 the expression of those views thus aid the consideration of alternatives at a group level.” Hence, although educational programs like the one evaluated for this study function at changing individuals’ attitudes and behaviors, individuals are perfectly capable of creating group-level change within their social group (i.e. team). As a last point, it has been found that “ingroup” minorities (e. g. teammates) are more effective than “outgroup” persons (e. g. non-teammates or prevention program facilitators) at altering group normative behavior or attitudes (Clark & Maas, 1988; Nemeth, 1995; Volpato et al., 1990). This underscores the importance that trainings involving athletes work to increase individual team member’s propensity to act with a minority dissenting opinion when involved in a situation that contradicts their own personal judgments. To increase the chances that an individual can actually change group norms of behavior, educational programs such as the one evaluated for this study should be designed to help individuals gain the knowledge and tools necessary to hold a consistent minority dissenting opinion when faced with group normative pressure. Only one sexual assault prevention program to date emphasizes the importance of minority dissent in the prevention of male sexual violence, and even so, the focus is on fraternity groups (Mahlstedt & Jacobson, unpublished manuscript 2000). In the Fraternity Violence Education Project, Mahlstedt & Jacobson (unpublished manuscript, 2000) explain that “the goal [of educating men to become minority dissenters] is that they become a rrrinority group that creates tension in the larger fraternity culture, provide an alternative model, will be present at the parties to influence the climate so that it will be a safe one for women, and intervene when necessary” (p. 1). The pressure that minority 43 groups and individuals exert on the group norms of a rape-supportive culture is exactly what changes the norms of that culture over time. 9mm Considering that there is now some evidence that college sport participation may have some connection to increased sexual aggression (Crosset, 1996), it is LISBfili to see if programs aimed at changing collegiate athletes’ attitudes and knowledge about sexual assault are making any kind of impact. The programs that have been implemented with athletes seem to be of good quality and philosophy and it is important to start investigating the effectiveness of such potentially groundbreaking programs. As with the numerous prevention and education programs targeting the general college student body, programs targeting student athletes need to be evaluated for their usefirlness so that efforts at improvement can be made and successfiil formats passed on to others. Perhaps it is even more important to explore programs targeting athletes, as student athletes’ schedules are extremely pressed for time, and more effective workshops will undoubtedly have more support from athletic department administrators and coaches. There are certain factors associated with an individual’s change in attitudes or knowledge due to an intervention that have been evaluated within the general rape prevention literature. For example, acceptance of interpersonal violence and hostility toward women are two of the factors most often associated with a person’s level of attitude or knowledge change. However, because student athletes have additional factors that may play a role in their level of sexual aggression (e. g. discouragement from identifying with anything considered “feminine” — extreme sex role socialization, high 44 need for achieving a goal or winning a competition) it is necessary to explore whether these additional factors also play a role in their attitude and knowledge change after participation in an educational intervention. Although they have not been as thoroughly investigated, many of these variables have been outlined above, in addition to a few that have been suggested by researchers as having an impact (e. g. contact versus non-contact sport participation). One of the most salient issues in working with a group culture such as collegiate athletes is how important it is to understand and acknowledge if intervention targeting a group-level change in behavior is possible. Considering the literature documenting the influence peer support has on individual behavior (Hackman, 1976), it is especially important to attempt change at the group, as well as the individual level. The current program that specifically targeted collegiate athletes was uniquely equipped with the exercises and environment that would make group change possible. One of the program’s goals was to give men the tools they need to confront peer pressure and act as minority dissenters, which is an essential component of any group-level effects. 45 Hypotheses The current study evaluated attitude and knowledge change in male student athletes after they had taken part in a sexual assault prevention and education program at a large mid-western university. The evaluation addressed the following research hypotheses: l. 2. Involvement in this program will decrease men’s reports of their rape myth acceptance and likelihood of sexually aggressing. Involvement in this program will increase men’s reports of their likelihood of confronting a situation in which there is sexual aggression. Effectiveness of the workshop (defined as significant positive change on rape myth acceptance, likelihood of sexually aggressing, and likelihood of confronting) is expected to be predicted by a number of men’s prior attitudes and experiences. Specifically, the workshop is expected to be less effective for those men who report pre-workshop: 3. 4. 8. 9. Higher goal orientation, win orientation, and competitiveness More extreme levels of prior sexual aggression Higher hostility toward women Higher sex role stereotyping Higher acceptance of interpersonal violence Higher acceptance of general violence Involvement in contact sports 10. More years participated in intercollegiate sports 46 Chapter 2 METHOD Sam 1e The workshop that was evaluated targeted all male student athletes at a large mid- westem university who at the time were not members of the freshman academic class. Ten program sessions were conducted, each of which was an hour and a half in duration. Approximately 180 peOple attended the program, and 150 evaluation packets were collected. Power analyses indicated that a minimum sample size of 125 would give power of .80 at p<.05 for all analyses used. Power analyses were conducted using PASS: Power Analysis and Sample Size (Version 6.0) Kaysville, UT: NC SS. More information about the power required for each analysis is available in the statistical analysis section at the end of this chapter. The Program Out of Bounds: An Anti—— Violence Workshop for Student Athletes at MS U The workshop was mandated by the Provost of the University to provide student athletes with education about sexual assault, sexual harassment, and relationship violence. In addition to sexual assault, other topics of abuse were added to the training content, to capitalize on the opportunity of having a training of this nature. A planning committee of campus stakeholders was formed to develop and implement the trainings with the student athletes. All of the people on the planning committee had multiple years 47 of experience in and/or knowledge about relationship violence education and prevention. Members included the following: representatives from the campus sexual assault counseling and crisis center (each with over fifteen years of experience), the director of the campus domestic violence shelter (over fifteen years of experience), two male outreach coordinators from the campus shelter and counseling program (over ten and four years of experience, respectively), three campus police officers (about four years of experience each), a representative from the campus women’s resource center (about seven years of experience), two members of the student athlete support services (non- athletic department), one program evaluator (author of this paper), as well as additional people involved with the facilitation of the workshops. Each workshop session was approximately one and a half hours in length, single- sex (all men). Having an all-male audience followed on the suggestion of previous researchers that a male majority environment lends for more open, safe, and honest discussion about the sensitive tOpic of sexual assault (e. g. Katz, 1995). Often male participants in mixed-sex workshops feel targeted by just the presence of women in the audience and the presentation of the issue of sexual assault. By keeping the participants in a single-sex environment, the chances for men being defensive and unwilling to participate due to other women athletes’ presence was hopefirlly lowered. Facilitators were men and women of varying ethnicities, and care was taken to ensure that all sessions included male facilitators. The workshop activities were developed by using a combination of reports from other college programs working with athletes, and the various programming experience of the members of the planning committee. A pilot workshop was held with all freshman 48 student athletes, and feedback from the evaluation of that session was also used to revise certain activities and program format. The current program’s workshops were held in an auditorium with tables and movable chairs, which made mobility of participants for activities easier than in most auditorium settings. The auditorium was also used as a study hall and team meeting room for the athletes on a regular basis, and therefore was known to the athletes as “their territory.” It was the desire of the planning comnrittee to hold the program in a setting that was well known to the athletes, to increase the chances of the program discussion being seen as something that mattered in their lives as an athlete, rather than as a separate and therefore forgettable event. The main goal of this workshop was to increase education about relationship violence, and help men learn ways to intervene with teammates or fiiends if they thought something inappropriate was occurring. The planning committee decided unanimously that a lack of “freely given consent” was the underlying common thread between all forms of intimate violence. The concept of “freely given consent” was understood by the facilitators as incorporating the following main points: 1. Consent is based on choice 2. Consent is active, not passive 3. Consent is possible only when there is equal power 4. Giving in because of fear is not consent 5. Going along with something because of wanting to fit in with the group, being deceived or feeling bad, is not consent 6. If you can’t say “no” comfortably, then “yes” has no meaning 7. If you are unwilling to accept a “no,” then “yes” has no meaning (Vanburen-Hay, 1999) As such, the whole framework of the presentation was centered on the concept of consent, and each of the points above were explained with examples to the audience at various times during the program. 49 At the beginning of the workshop, a brief introduction video was shown to the participants. The video was professionally produced for this particular group of presentations, as an effort to not only capture participants’ attention from the start, but also as an explanation of the importance of their attendance at the workshop that day. The video featured a former football star from the university who later played professional football before becoming a campus police officer at the university these sessions were held. With a backdrop of contemporary music, video clips of sporting events, and newspaper clippings from past stories of athletes who were arrested or accused of committing intimate partner violence, the officer explained the special role that athletes have in their community and the importance of them taking their prestige seriously. It was explained that not only did their prestige on the college campus and in the community buy them privileges; their fame also focused the spotlight of media attention to personal wrongdoings more intensely than on other students at the university. He thanked them for coming, and urged their attention throughout the workshop. After the video, the facilitators introduced themselves and reviewed an overhead of the five main points of the workshop: 1. Definitions 2. Consent 3. Laws 4. Choices 5. Resources It was explained to the participants that each person in the room had the ability to make his own decisions, and every action he took as an individual was his own choice, despite how much group pressure he felt. In addition, it was emphasized that every choice has good and/or bad consequences, and that more discussion about consequences and ways to 50 intervene would be explained later in the program. A brief explanation of how each main point would be addressed throughout the program activities was explained by one of the facilitators. After this initial introduction, participants raised their hands as a group to signify on what athletic team they participated. Certain workshops had more than one team present, so the planning committee felt it was important to have the players be familiar with each other enough to know that they were accounted for and not invisible to the group or the facilitators. A facilitator, at this point, explained what the purpose of this workshop was, and the importance of talking about sexual assault. They also acknowledged that some people in the room may have already been abused, either as children or adults, and that sexual abuse is not only a crime against women. Because attendance at workshops ranged between 30-75 participants per workshop, the decision was made to incorporate interactive discussion-based activities. The first activity was the “Ball Toss,” in which the participants were seated as a larger group. One of the facilitators stood on the front stage and threw a miniature soft ball to the crowd. Whoever caught the ball responded to the question “How do you know if someone wants to kiss you?” The first person gave his response and then threw it to another person in the audience who answered the same question. This process repeated for about six responses, and then the ball was thrown back to the facilitator for discussion. About three questions were asked and answered in this manner as an icebreaker and warm up for the group. This activity also encouraged participation right fiom the start of the workshop and sensitized the participants to the topics of sexual assault and relationship violence. 51 The next activity was a discussion about the myths and facts of intimate partner violence. There were a wide variety of myths and facts displayed on individual overheads, with the sexual assault myths paralleling the myths used on the pre and post- test questionnaires. For each myth, participants were asked to raise their hand signifying if they thought it was true or false. The complimentary “fact” slide was shown after the “myth” slide. The facilitator discussed each myth with individuals who brought up concerns. The next part of the program consisted of a short video clip and discussion about consent. The video clip was a scene from the recent popular series “Party of Five,” in which one of the college age female characters is involved in an attempted rape by her current boyfiiend at a party. After the video, facilitators engaged the audience in a discussion about what happened in the video and whether or not she freely gave consent to have sex. This activity usually had the most interaction, as many people had opinions about the woman’s behavior in the situation and whether or not drinking (which the characters were doing at the party) affected someone’s ability to give or receive consent. After the consent piece was completed, participants were presented with a short computer overhead presentation about the laws regarding criminal sexual conduct, domestic violence, and sexual harassment. Questions were taken from the group. The last exercise was another discussion-based interactive activity that was aimed at helping men decide what their options for action were when faced with being a bystander in a situation where something sexually abusive was (or would soon be) occurring. Three scenarios were developed specifically about situations where male athletes may be involved. This activity was based on a similar, yet different exercise 52 created by Katz (1995), which has been used by the Mentors in Violence Prevention Project. The scenarios were real-life situations that most men have experienced, or at least known about in their families or communities. All scenarios were read from the bystander’s perspective, not as a perpetrator or victim. As mentioned previously, it is essential in a workshop with student athletes to discuss the issue of being a bystander to situations that might bring up ethical and moral concerns for a person. Perhaps the most salient reason for targeting athletes for prevention/education trainings is the research documenting that persons who would not typically victimize a person by themselves are often pressured into doing so in a group situation heavily guided by male bonding and team membership (Boeringer, 1999). Asking participants to consider their opinions about what they would and possibly should do when faced with group pressure is essential to tap into this phenomenon. For example, one of the scenarios was “You see a teammate of yours with a woman who is dancing very provocatively and appears to be really drunk. After they finish dancing, he grabs her by the hand to lead her upstairs. She doesn’t seem like she is into it and appears to be having trouble standing and walking.” After reading the scenario out loud, the facilitator walked participants through their thought processes if they were actually in that situation. Steps included a discussion about the following: 1. Facts of the scenario (e. g. he is your teammate, she is drunk) 2. His options (for this scenario, options usually included walking away, intervening between the two of them, taking your teammate to the side and telling him what he’s doing is wrong, etc.) 53 3. The consequences of his decision for each of the options presented (e. g. she is not assaulted, your teammate may get upset, she may be assaulted and you witnessed it enough to be prosecuted, you may feel stupid in front of your teammates). This exercise always opened up a lively discussion about a person’s options for action when he is faced with tough, often peer influenced decisions in real life. After the scenarios were completed, facilitators explained what campus resources were available to participants and/or anyone in their lives who may need help with any of the issues discussed in the program. Evaluation The evaluation consisted of a pre-test and post-test that were given matching numbers to ensure the ability to match them in the event that they became separated. One pre-test and one post-test were placed into a manila envelope, which was labeled with the same identifying number. A cover sheet on both the pre-test and post-test explained that participation in the survey was voluntary, and that all information would be kept strictly confidential. A similar cover sheet was used for both test administrations, and explained that the project was interested in learning more about students’ perceptions about relationships and themselves. Prior to the start of the program, the pre—test and the post- test were placed in an envelope and set on the tables at which the participants sat for the duration of the presentation. Care was taken to ensure that there was ample space between participants to ensure confidentiality. All questionnaires were filled out in the room in which the presentation was held. As participants entered the room for the training, a facilitator at the entrance greeted them and instructed them to take a seat where 54 there was already an envelope and start filling out the green packet (pre-test). The remaining facilitators stood at the front of the room to ensure that no one spoke or looked at the post-test, and that each person kept his answers to himself. As soon as people were finished filling out the green packet, they placed them back into the envelope and pushed the envelope to the side until the presentation was over. The entire pre-test took each person between 15 and 20 minutes. To minimize any coercion a participant would feel to complete the test packet, no coaches or persons with athletic department authority were present in the room. After the presentation content was completed, one of the facilitators introduced the principal investigator of the evaluation, who then explained to participants the process of completing the post-test. The evaluator attended and explained the post-test procedure at every presentation to ensure that participants were given identical information at each presentation. Participants filled out the post-test in the same fashion in which they completed the pre-test, placed it into the envelope, and dropped the envelope in a box by the door on their way out. As participants left the room, they were given a sheet with a list of resources and information about obtaining counseling, which was meant to minimize individuals’ embarrassment or uneasiness at having to request such resources. The decision to hand out these sheets at the door was made to ensure that those people who opted to not complete the survey would still receive the list of resources and information. A file was created for each participant in a locked file cabinet at the university that was only accessible to the project’s investigators and research assistants. No names or any other identifying information were required or collected. Only the numbers printed on the pre-test and post-test in each packet with no identifying 55 information were entered into the data set. Post-test completion time ranged from 10-15 rrrinutes, and procedures for ensuring confidentiality were the same as with the pre-test conditions. In addition to administering the pre and post-tests, the principal investigator attended each of the sessions to observe the interactions of the audience and facilitators, as well as to observe the overall tone of each presentation. As noted later in the discussion section of this paper, the researcher was able to observe the apparent receptiveness of participants to their attendance at the workshop, in addition to their overall response to the pre and post-test. The researcher was only visible to the audience at the end of the presentation, when she explained the procedures related to the completion of the post-test. Otherwise, the researcher stayed at the back of the room, far away from any of the participants to eliminate the possibility that participants felt they were being “watched.” Measurement Ten measures were used in this study. Demographics, life experiences, and attitudinal scales related to predictor variables were included only in the pre-test packet because they were not a target for change via the workshop. Pre-test attitudinal variables included intra-individual factors that have typically been measured in rape prevention program evaluations (e. g. sex role stereotyping, hostility toward women), in addition to sport-specific factors that have not yet been measured in the context of an evaluation (e. g. competitiveness). Post-test attitudinal variables included all outcome variables that were a target for change via the workshop. Also included in the post-test were group- 56 utilization variables that tested whether or not group-level change was possibly achieved as a result of the workshop. Demoggrphics Data was collected on the type of sport played (e. g. contact/non—contact), and number of years the student participated as an intercollegiate athlete. These measures addressed the environmental factors that may have played a role in not only a man’s sexual aggression, but also his level of attitude change due to the intervention. Contact vs. Non-Contact Sport Participation All sports offered as intercollegiate varsity sports at the participating university that were classified as non-contact were listed in a box, and participants were asked to check the box if it contained the sport they participated in as an intercollegiate athlete. Teams classified as non-contact included: tennis, volleyball, cross country, track & field, golf, baseball, swimming, gymnastics and crew. Contact sports included: football, basketball, wrestling, soccer, and ice hockey. To ensure confidentiality, participants were not asked their specific team membership, as it would have been difficult to ensure anonymity when reporting results on a team-by-team basis. Competitiveness, Goal Orientation, Win Orientation Competitiveness, Goal Orientation, and Win Orientation were measured using the Sport Orientation Questionnaire (SOQ) developed by Gill & Deeter (1988). Original reliability analyses on each of the three subscales (win orientation, goal orientation, and competitiveness) indicated alpha coefficients of .72, .78, .91, respectively (Gill & Deeter, 1988). The alpha coefficient for the combined three subscales was .92. The questionnaire was developed as a “sport-specific multi-dimensional measure of 57 achievement orientation” (Gill & Deeter, 1988, p. 192). It captures not only an individual’s motivation for achievement within the world of sports, but also a person’s level of interpersonal achievement. The authors suggest that such personal qualities as competitiveness and achievement orientation may easily cross over from sports into an individual’s personal life. Respondents were asked to indicate how strongly they agreed with 25 statements about winning (6 items), goal attainment (6 items), and competitiveness (13 items) on a scale ranging from 1 (strongly agree) to 5 (strongly disagree). Examples of statements included on the questionnaire were: “I am a competitive person,” “Losing upsets me,” “I look forward to the opportunity to test my skills in competition,” and “The best way to determine my ability is to set a goal and try to reach it.” For the current study, reliability analyses on each of the three subscales (win orientation, goal orientation, and competitiveness) indicated alpha coefficients of .79, .72, .93, respectively. Range of corrected item-to-total correlations are listed below in Table 1. The alpha coefficient for the combined three subscales was .93. The mean scores for each of the three subscales (or the mean score of the entire scale with all three dimensions included) were utilized for analyses in this study. Table 1 Sport Orientation Subscale Item-to-Total Correlations # Items Lowest Item-to- Highest Item-to- Coefficient in total Correlation total Correlation Alpha Subscale Win orientation 6 .36 .71 .79 Goal Orientation 6 .42 .57 .72 Competitiveness 13 .56 .84 .93 58 Attitudes Toward General Violence and Interpersonal Violence Respondents’ general attitudes toward violence, as well as their attitudes toward interpersonal violence were both measured by the Attitudes Toward Violence Scale developed by Lonsway & Fitzgerald (1995). This scale had recently been used in rape prevention evaluations, as a result of Lonsway & Fitzgerald’s (1995) finding that other attitudinal scales commonly used (e.g. hostility toward women) confounded a respondent’s attitudes toward violence. This scale more accurately differentiates a respondent’s acceptance of general violence from his or her acceptance of interpersonal violence in a way that is not as confounded by other attitudinal variables (e. g. hostility toward women). Sample items included “Violent crimes should be punished violently,” “The government should send armed soldiers to control violent university riots,” “Giving mischievous children a quick slap is the best way to quickly end trouble,” and “It is alright for a partner to hit the other if they are unfaithfirl.” Respondents indicated using a scale of 1 (not at all agree) to 7 (very much agree) how much they agreed with each statement. Lonsway & Fitzgerald reported an or of .87 for the entire scale, which consists of 20 items (10 general violence, 10 interpersonal violence). Corrected item-to-total correlations for the entire 20-item scale ranged from .23 to .65. Although the questions were split equally between conceptual constructs, the original developers of the scale did not analyze or report subscale psychometrics. To explore the possibility of analyzing this scale as two separate subscales, factor analyses and reliability tests were run. Exploratory factor analysis with varimax rotation yielded three factors. The first factor included the items measuring acceptance of violence against children, the second factor included items measuring acceptance of violence against intimate partners, and the 59 third factor included items measuring the acceptance of general violence. Since this scale was developed to measure two factors, including acceptance of general violence and acceptance of interpersonal violence, reliability tests were run to determine if the first two factors could be analyzed as one subscale, despite their possible separation into two factors. The alpha coefficient for the first two factors together was .88 (corrected item- to-total correlations ranged from .40 to .78). The alpha coefficient for the third factor was .77 (corrected item-to-total correlations ranged from .21 to .55). Since it was conceptually desirable to use the first two factors together as a measure of acceptance of interpersonal violence, the decision was made to use the mean score of the first two factors to create one subscale that measured acceptance of interpersonal violence. The mean score of the last factor was used to create the second subscale that measured acceptance of general violence. Sex Role Stereotyping Sex role stereotyping was measured using the Sex Role Stereotyping Scale developed by Burt (1980). This 9-item scale was created for the purpose of measuring the degree to which a person assigns men and women to traditional sex-specific roles. In Burt’s (1980) study, Cronbach’s alpha was reported as .80. Example statements included: “It is acceptable for the woman to pay for the date” and “A wife should never contradict her husband in public.” Respondents indicated using a scale of 1 (not at all agree) to 7 (very much agree) how much they agreed with each statement. Three items were reverse coded, and reliability analyses indicated an alpha coefficient of .60. Corrected item-to-total correlations ranged from .16 to .43. The mean score of all nine items was used for analyses in this study. 60 Hostility Toward Women Hostility toward women was measured by the Hostility Toward Women Scale, which was originally developed by Check, Malamuth, Elias, and Barton (1985), and later modified by Lonsway & Fitzgerald (1995). Coefficient alpha for this 10-item scale was reported by Lonsway and Fitzgerald (1995) as .83. A list often statements was used to assess a person’s level of hostility toward women. Respondents indicated using a scale of 1 (not at all agree) to 7 (very much agree) how much they agreed with each statement. Examples of statements included “I believe that most women tell the truth,” “I am easily angered by women,” and “Sometimes women bother me just by being around.” Two items were reverse coded, and the alpha coefficient for all items was .83. Corrected item- to-total correlations in this study ranged from .37 to .68. The mean score of all ten items was used for analyses in this study. Sexual Aggxession Severity A respondent’s severity of prior sexual aggression was measured by a revised 14- item version of the original ll-item version of the Sexual Experiences Survey that was developed by Koss et al. in 1985 and revised by Koss & Gaines in 1993. Internal consistency ratings for the original SES (Koss et al., 1985) were reported at .89, with test- retest agreement between l-week apart test administrations reported to be 93%. The 1993 version includes more concise wording of items and incorporates a wider variety of sexually coercive and abusive behaviors (including not just individually coercive behaviors, but also incorporating gang rapes). For example, questions were added asking if the respondent had ever made “catcalls” (i.e. sexual harassment) to a woman or had ever stood in line to have sex with a “party girl” (i.e. gang rape). Two additional 61 questions were added for the current study that specifically asked about a respondent’s involvement with “groupies,” since these women are often prominent figures in an athlete’s social life. These two questions asked how often a respondent has “had sex with a groupie” and “stood in line to have sex with a groupie.” Using a 5-point scale ranging from “Never” to “Often”, respondents indicated how often since they were 14 years old they had engaged in each of the listed behaviors. Dependent Vaiables Three instruments were used to measure the three dependent variables including 1) Rape Myth Acceptance, 2) Likelihood of Sexually Aggressing (i.e. Likelihood to Use Force, Likelihood to Rape, Likelihood to Use Coercion, Likelihood of Using Alcohol), and 3) Likelihood of Confronting someone in or after a sexually aggressive situation. Group (Team) Utilization was also measured at post-test, as an attempt to capture the possibility that group change (i.e. across a team) would occur after the presentation. Since the three dependent variables measured the outcome of the intervention, all of them, with the exception of the group utilization items, were measured at both pre and post-test. Group utilization, described later in this section, was measured only at post- test. For the remaining five outcome variables, scale construction was conducted independently with pre and post-test scores. These variables were measured as follows: 62 QflMXth Acceptance Rape myth acceptance was measured using the Illinois Rape Myth Acceptance Scale — Short Form (IRMA-SF), developed by Payne, Lonsway, and Fitzgerald in 1999. This scale was developed as a more accurate measure of rape myth acceptance than Burt’s (1980) original Rape Myth Acceptance Scale. Coefficient alpha for the scale constructed by Lonsway & Fitzgerald (1994) was reported as .87 (corrected item-to-total correlations ranged from .34 to .65). The definition of rape myths that are measured using the IRMA-SF are an expanded version of Burt’s (1980) original cultural definition of rape myths, and include multiple dimensions of rape myth acceptance. The IRMA-SF measures the extent to which a respondent adheres to rape myths, defined as “attitudes and beliefs that are generally false but are widely and persistently held, and that serve to deny and justify male sexual aggression against women,” (Lonsway & Fitzgerald, 1994, p. 134). Sample statements include “It is usually only women who dress suggestively who are raped” and “Rape happens when a man’s sex drive gets out of control.” Respondents indicated how much they agreed with each statement using a scale of 1 (not at all agree) to 7 (very much agree). For both pre and post-test scores, three items were reverse coded, and the mean was taken of all items on each test occasion to create a scale score. Alpha coefficient for the pre-test was .82 and .86 for the post-test. Corrected item-to-total correlations ranged from -. 18 to .76 for the pre-test and from -.11 to .74 for the post-test. 63 Behavioral Intentions (Likelihood to Use F orce, Likelihood to Rape, Likelihood to Use Coercion, Likelihood of UsingAlcohol. Likelihood to Confi'ont) All behavioral intentions were measured as part of a questionnaire developed by Berg, Lonsway, and Fitzgerald (1999). The first two behavioral intentions, Likelihood to Use Force (LF) and Likelihood to Rape (LR), were originally developed by Briere and Malamuth (1983), and reworded to their current form by Berg et al. (1999). Two of the other behavioral intentions, Likelihood to Use Coercion (LC) and Likelihood to Confront (LCon) were originally developed by Berg et al (1999). All items measuring the first three behavioral intentions (LF, LR, LC) were included as statements within a list of various sexual behaviors and actions. The fourth variable, Likelihood of Using Alcohol (LA), was created for the current study in response to the growing number of studies that have found alcohol and/or other substance use as significant predictors of sexual aggression or sexual force among both athletes as well as non-athletes (Boeringer, 1996; K038 and Gaines, 1993; Schwartz & Nogrady, 1996). There were a total of eleven statements, seven of which were distracter items. Respondents rated on a scale of 1 (never) to 5 (always) how often they would engage in each behavior if they could be assured that no one would know. Higher scores indicated an individual’s higher self-reported likelihood of engaging in sexually coercive behaviors (Berg et al., 1999). Although previous researchers have reported each of the behavioral intentions as a separate variable, it was a possible reliability concern that each intention was measured by using only a one-item statement. For example, the construct “Likelihood to Rape” has typically been measured only by the following statement: “If you could be assured that no one would know, how often would you force a woman to 64 have sex when she doesn’t want to?” Other researchers have just explicitly asked a man if he would rape if he was assured no one would know and he would not be punished (Boeringer, 1999). The other behavioral intention measures consisted of similar statements that capture the variety of ways that men may attempt to engage in sexual behavior with an unwilling woman (e. g. coercion, force). An exploratory factor analysis using varimax rotation was used to determine if these items would be more valid as several indicators of a central construct of “Likelihood of Sexually Aggressing.” This analysis indicated that at pre-test, all four items loaded highly on one component (.81, .76, .82, .86) and accounted for 66% of the total variance. At post-test, the same four items loaded even higher on the first component (.93, .96, .98, .90) and accounted for 89% of the total variance. Additionally, reliability tests with pre-test data indicated that the four items taken together as one scale resulted in an alpha coefficient of .82 (corrected item-to—total correlations ranging from .58 to .72), while the post-test alpha coefficient was .96 (corrected item-to-total correlations ranging from .87 to .95). Therefore, for conceptual ease of purpose, as well as psychometric clarity, the mean of the scores for each of the four behavioral intention measures (likelihood to use force for sexual acts, force for intercourse, coercion, and alcohol), was used to create a scale score measuring the construct of “likelihood of sexually aggressing.” The fifth behavioral intention measure, Likelihood to Confront, was designed by Berg et al., (1999) “to assess the level of confrontation that participants would engage in when they have reason to believe that a woman is being made to do something that she does not want to do” (Berg et al., 1999, p. 224). Respondents were asked to respond to 65 six scenarios by choosing one of four different options. Relationships of either the person instigating the behavior or receiving the behavior were varied. For example, the person instigating the sexual behavior was either the respondent’s “teammate” or “ a man” (i.e. stranger), and the woman in the scenario was either “a good fiiend” of his or someone he “does not know.” The use of the relationship of a “teammate” in the scenario was reworded for the current study from “roommate” to “teammate” in order to make the question more relevant to student athletes’ experiences of being involved in a team culture. In addition, different intensities of sexual acts were also varied for each scenario (e. g. kissing or making her do “something sexual” she doesn’t want to do). Response options ranged from “do nothing” to “interrupting the situation to see if everything is alright.” Although each scenario was created to be conceptually unique from the other scenarios, reliability analyses indicated that all six items held together as one scale measuring a person’s “likelihood to confront” (scale coefficient alpha = .74; corrected item-to-total correlations ranged from .40 to .68). For ease of multivariate analyses, all six items were combined to make one scale measuring the outcome variable “likelihood to confront.” This scale was used in all multivariate analyses for this study. However, items were also analyzed separately (i.e. not as a scale) for descriptive purposes. Descriptive analyses are discussed later in the results section. Program Utilization A selection of questions gauged at measuring projected group-level change (within each team), as well as individual utilization of program information was also developed for this study. The questions developed for this study followed the concept of 66 a type of evaluative technique termed “utilization-focused evaluation” (Patton, 1990). This final collection of “group utilization” outcome variables is unique fi'om the other outcome variables for this study because it was only measured at post-test, rather than at both pre-and post-test. One of the purposes of the utilization-focused evaluation technique is to ensure that the event that is evaluated (e. g. an informational workshop) is actually relevant to the population that is expected to use the information from the event (Patton, 1990). Although this type of evaluation is often considered a process that requires participant participation throughout the entire evaluation planning and implementation (Patton, 1990), for this study, only the practicality of this concept was used, so program participants were not directly involved in the development of evaluation questions. For example, the practicality of asking participants how much they will use the information that was provided at the workshop was what was deemed important for the current study, whereas in other studies the participants might have been involved in the practicality of the evaluation itself, rather than just the program inforrnation’s practicality to them. The group utilization items for this study focused on exploring whether or not the information provided by the program was meaningful and usefirl to the program participants. In this sense, this technique offers a very practical way of asking how useful the workshop was for participants, as well as a check on the accuracy of findings from pre-post test measures. For example, since the current study evaluated change by administering pre and post-tests only an hour apart, it was a good additional measure to find out if participants directly believed that the information presented was something they would put to use or not, regardless of attitudinal or abstract pre-post test attitudinal 67 measures. The practicality of just asking participants directly whether or not they will use the program information is regarded as one of the strengths of the utilization-focused evaluation approach. Patton (1990, p. 405) states, “A utilization-focused approach to analysis can help keep the findings from becoming too abstract, esoteric, or theoretical.” The level of program usefulness that the program participants report is an important and direct way of knowing whether or not the workshop information will actually be used and make a diflerence in their lives. Aside from exploring individuals’ use of the information, utilization questions for this study were used to help determine the likelihood that other team members would use the information. Asking an individual team member how much his team would use the information from the workshop goes a step farther into a participant’s estimation of group-level change. As noted earlier, no other program targeting athletes has attempted to measure whether change at the group level of interaction may occur as a result of an intervention (Katz, 1995; Parrot et al., 1994). Other ways of determining group-level change (e. g. participant observation of group behaviors, focus groups held months after the intervention) were impractical for this type of study. Therefore, the decision to use utilization questions was made to ensure that at least some estimation of group behavior was attained. General program usefulness. Items developed for this study measured varying components of behavioral impact. For example, to find out the general usefulness of such a presentation (and therefore likelihood of making an actual behavioral impact) the question was asked “How much do you think you will use some of the information that was presented in this workshop?” To capture not only individual utilization, but also the 68 possibility of team utilization, the question was asked “How much do you think members of your team will use some of the information that was presented in this workshop?” Respondents answered both of these questions using a scale ranging from 1 “very much” to 4 “not much at all.” Blank space for clarification on both of these questions was also available to respondents (asked in the form of the question “what makes you say that?”). All open-ended responses for individual utilization items were reviewed and then coded into six emergent categories of response by the researcher (see Table 2). Two coders were trained in the use of the coding categories, then independently read and coded the responses according to the predetermined categories. Initially, out of sixty-three coded responses, coders agreed on fifty-nine. Thus, initial inter-coder agreement was 94%. Table 2 Individual Utilization Coding Categories (n = 65) Coding Category Example of response included in category . No new information/ common sense (n = 29) “I know most of it” “Because I didn’t learn anything new” “It’s common sense!” . These situations do not happen often (11 = 7) “Not confronted with it a lot” “Hasn’t happened yet” “Don’t go to places it would happen” . Would behave in the ways suggested by the program anyways (n = 4) “I already would do that” “Cuz I’m not gonna sexually harass anyone” (sic) . Would not behave in the ways suggested by the program anyways (n = 3) “I wouldn’t do it anyway” “Because sexual assault will always be present” . New information (11 = 15) “It’s good to know the definitions” “There is some information I did not know” . These situations happen often (n = 4) “I’m in these situations often” “A lot of these instances occur” “It happens a lot (these situations)” 69 As with the individual utilization items, all open—ended responses for team utilization items were reviewed and then coded into six emergent categories of response by the researcher (see Table 3). Two coders were trained in the use of the coding categories, then independently read and coded the responses according to the predetermined categories. Twelve responses were thrown out because they did not answer the question, were incomplete, or did not make sense to either of the coders. Initially, out of fifty-five coded responses, coders agreed on fifty-one. Thus, initial inter- coder agreement was 93%. Table 3 Team Utilization Coding Categories (n = 55) Coding Category Example of response included in category 1. Lack of attentiveness or “I don’t think a lot of my teammates would caring for program care” material (n = 16) “They all thought it was stupid we had to attend this meeting” “It all depends on how well they paid attention” 2. Information was “common “We learned this is third grade” (sic) sense” (n = 9) “Common sense” “We all know” 3. Team is inherently bad “There are some dirty pigs on my team” (n = 6) “We’re gross!” “They do not care. NO respect” “They are pretty bad guys who think they deserve it all” 4. Team lacks intelligence “Because they are idiots!” (n = 5) “Some of them aren’t very smart” “Their jocks” (sic) 5. New information (n = 10) “It was informative” “Because they learned” “It seemed like everyone got something from what was said” 6. Team is inherently good “We don’t do this crap” (n = 9) “We are a good team” 70 Confrontation with a teammate. Two items were created to more directly determine if the intervention helped participants receive the behavioral or attitudinal tools they would need to assume the role of a minority dissenter. As a follow-up to the scenario questions involving confrontation (described above), the first item asked respondents “What factors would influence your decision to confront a teammate who was making a woman do something sexual that she does not want to do?” All open- ended responses were reviewed and then coded into seven emergent categories of response by the researcher (see Table 4). Two coders were trained in the use of the coding categories, then independently read and coded the responses according to the predetermined categories. Eleven responses were thrown out because they did not answer the question, were incomplete, or did not make sense to any of the coders. Initially, out of seventy-five coded responses, coders agreed on sixty-six. Thus, initial inter-coder agreement was 88%. Coding categories were fiirther discussed between the researcher and the two coders and revised so that all three were in agreement with the defining features of each category. Responses were again independently reviewed by each coder, and as a result, coders agreed on seventy-three out of the seventy-five responses, indicating a 97% inter-coder agreement. An additional item was also added that asked respondents “How much do you think the information that was presented to you in this workshop would help you stand up to a teammate if he was doing something with a woman that you thought was wrong?” For this item, respondents indicated on a scale ranging from “won’t help me at all in any situation” to “will help me in all situations,” in addition to one answer choice “I already 71 would have stood up to a teammate in any situation.” Respondents who answered that it would not help them in any situation were asked to briefly explain why they didn’t think the information would help them. Very few respondents (n= 7) answered this last open- ended question, so it was not used in any of the analyses for this study. Table 4 Coding Categories for Factors Influencing Participants’ Confrontation of a Teammate (n = 75*) Coding Category Example of response included in category 1. Cues from the woman “The way the female is acting” (n = 19) “If she was upset or mad” “If she was screaming help or something else” “Amcunt of resistance by the woman” 2. Situational factors “Environment and time” (n = 15) “Atmosphere and setting and extent of the situation” “Where it is at” 3. Protecting teammate “If he was doing something criminal” (n = 13) “If he was going to get himself in trouble” “Don’t want him to get in trouble” 4. Personal morals “I don’t care who it is, I don’t think it’s right” (n = 12) “It is not right” “Rights of everyone — it’s common sense” 5. Relationship with teammate “Who the teammate was” (n = 12) “How well you know him, if they’re violent” “How close of a friend he is” 6. Relationship with woman “How well I know her” (11 = 9) “If she was my sister or relative, someone close” “Who the woman was, if she could handle herself or not” 7. Cues from teammate “The way he is touching her” (n = 8) “If he was forceful” “If they’re violent” *n = 73 responses, but some responses listed multiple factors. The total number of factors given was 11 = 93. 72 Statistical Analyses Multivagiate Analyses To test the hypotheses that involvement in this program would decrease men’s reports of their rape myth acceptance and likelihood of sexually aggressing and increase men’s reports of their likelihood of confronting a situation, repeated measures multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) was used to initially compare pre and post- test scores. Power analyses indicated that for MAN OVA, N =125 (answering both pre and post-tests) gave power = .80 to find pre-post change significant at p < .05 if the effect size is at least as big as .25 standard deviations. Power analyses were conducted using PASS: Power Analysis and Sample Size (Version 6.0) Kaysville, UT: NCSS. When preparing to run the repeated measures MANOVA, it became apparent that there were a number of respondents who were listwise deleted from the analysis. Case summaries were run to determine what cases were missing from the dataset, which revealed that the majority of cases that were listwise deleted consisted of seventeen football players and ten members of others teams who did not complete their entire post- test. Although these players had completed most scales on their post-test, because they were missing one or more scales, listwise deletion procedures (used in general multivariate analyses) deleted them from all analyses. To more accurately estimate the values of this missing data, an expectation maximization procedure was run. Expectation maximization (EM) is an estimating procedure that is known for producing more realistic estimates of variance than other methods of missing data estimation (e. g. mean imputation) (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2000). Tabachnick and Fidell (2000) explain: “EM forms a missing data correlation (or 73 covariance) matrix by assuming the shape of a distribution (such as normal) for the partially missing data and basing inferences about missing values on the likelihood under that distribution” (p.63). By using estimates based on the distribution of existing data, this procedure of estimating missing data not only more accurately reflects the actual sample population, but also avoids impossible matrices and overfitting (making the estimation look better than it actually is). The results of t-tests that were run to determine if cases in which data were estimated were different than cases without estimated data are provided at the beginning of the results section. A total of 60 out of 720 values (or 8.33% of the data matrix) were estimated using EM procedures. Predictors of Post-Intervention Changa To examine the relationships between predictor variables and participants’ post- scores, a sequential regression (also called hierarchical regression) of predictors on post scores was conducted for each outcome variable. Pre-scores were co-varied out of the equation by entering them into the first block of each regression equation. Sample size for all sequential regressions was N = 120, which yielded only slightly lower power than had been anticipated. For sequential regression analyses, power analyses indicated that N =125 should give power = .80 at (p < .05) to find significant an R square change = .05 when pre-scores account for 25% of the variance (implying pre-post stability, or the correlation between pre and post-scores = .5). Power analyses were conducted using PASS: Power Analysis and Sample Size (Version 6.0) Kaysville, UT: NCSS. All data were entered into and analyzed with the Statistical Package for Social Scientists (SPSS), version 10.0. A total of 1 16 out of 1920 values (or 6.04% of the data matrix) were estimated using EM procedures. 74 Chapter 3 RESULTS Of the 150 survey packets collected, 120 were used for analyses in the current study. Six surveys were deleted from the dataset due to a lack of information on both the pre and post-test. Twenty-four surveys could not be used for one or more of the following reasons: 1. Incomplete post-test (less than one page completed). n = 4 2. Incomplete pre-test (less than one page completed). n = 3 3. Answers to one or more of the measurements were clearly fabricated or answered with sarcasm. n = 17 Cases that fell under the last criterion above were determined through inspection of numerical answers to items on the scales included in either the pre or post-test. Items that were numerically answered with an obvious pattern were included under this third criterion for exclusion. For example, if all twenty-five items on a scale were marked as “undecided” and/or answers increased numerically in order from lowest option to highest, and then repeated, such as 1,2,3,4,5,6,7,1,2,3,4,5,6,7. . .), or if items were answered without regard to the reverse coded items (e.g. answering the reverse coded items within the same range as the non-reverse coded items were answered), the case was excluded from analyses. Surveys that fell under the third criterion for exclusion above at either the pre-test or the post-test were excluded from analyses. This decision was made with the assumption that scores that were fabricated or answered sarcastically by a person on 75 either the pre or post-test could not be assuredly deemed as reliable on the opposite test administration. Independent sample t-tests indicated that people who were excluded from analyses using the above selection criteria were significantly difi’erent than people who were included in analyses. As Table 5 indicates, participants who were excluded from analyses had slightly higher sex role stereotyping (excluded participant mean score: 3.68; included mean scale score: 3.10) and severity of prior sexual aggression (excluded mean scale score: 1.87; included mean scale score: 1.27) than participants who were included in the analyses. Table 5 Independent Sample T-Tests for Cases Excluded From Analyses Based on Selection Criteria Variable T df Sig. (two-tailed) Rape Myth Accemnce -.307 142 .759 Likelihood of Sexually -l .849 142 .067 Aggressing Acceptance of -1.466 142 .145 Interpersonal Violence Likelihood of Confronting .763 142 .446 Acceptance of General -1.636 142 .104 Violence Contact vs. Non-Contact .298 142 .766 Sport Participation Hostility Toward Women -.471 142 .639 Sex Role Stereotyping —2.392 142 .018 Severity of Sexual -2.386 142 .018 Aggression Competitiveness -.011 142 .992 Goal Orientation .451 142 .653 Win Orientation .418 142 .677 Years played -.553 109 .581 intercollegiate sports 76 Independent sample t-tests were also run to determine if cases for which post-test data were estimated (through EM procedures) were significantly different from non- estimated cases. T-tests indicated that people who did not completely fill out their post- tests were significantly different from the group that did complete the post-test on only one of the thirteen variables. As Table 6 indicates, participants who did not complete their entire post-test scored significantly higher on acceptance of interpersonal violence than participants who completed the entire post-test (uncompleted post-test mean scale score: 2.97; completed post—test mean scale score: 2.47). Table 6 Independent Sample T-Tests for Estimated Cases Based on Missing Partial Post-Test Variable T df Sig. (two-tailed) Rape Myth Acceptance .056 142 .955 Likelihood of Sexually .883 142 .379 Aggressing Acceptance of -2.008 142 .047 Interpersonal Violence Likelihood of Confronting .710 142 .479 Acceptance of General -.704 142 .483 Violence Contact vs. Non-Contact 1.287 142 .200 Sport Participation Hostility Toward Women -1.240 142 .217 Sex Role Stereotyping -1 .7 87 142 .076 Severity of Sexual -.7 96 142 .427 Aggression Competitiveness -. 170 142 .865 Goal Orientation -.430 142 .668 Win Orientation -.142 142 .887 Years played -.812 109 .419 intercollegiate sports 77 Pre-Test Descriptives F ifty-seven percent of the sample were members of a contact-sport, and forty- three percent were members of a non-contact sport. Respondents ranged between one and four years of collegiate sports participation, with most having played for one year (33%). Twenty-three percent had participated for two years, 12% had participated for three years, and 9% had participated for four years or more. Twenty-three percent of the respondents did not answer how many years they played sports. This is most likely due to the location of the question on the questionnaire page (very top of second page), which probably made it easier to skip than subsequent questions. Descriptive findings for variables measured on the same 7-point scale are listed in Table 7. Overall, respondents were somewhat less supportive of the use of interpersonal violence than they were of general violence. However, the majority of responses indicated that respondents were generally unsupportive of all types of violence. Respondents generally reported low hostility toward women, although only 3% indicated that they did not at all agree with statements endorsing hostility toward women. The majority of respondents (66%) reported that they mostly or somewhat did not agree with statements indicating hostility toward women. Respondents were also generally unsupportive of sex role stereotypes. Only seven percent of respondents indicated that they at least somewhat agreed with sex role stereotypes, and not one person indicated that they very much agreed with the statements. Twenty-five percent of respondents were undecided about the statements. 78 Table 7 Predictor Variable Scale Percentages at Pre-Test (measured on a 7—point scale] l 2 3 4 5 6 7 . Scale Variable Not Mostly Some- Un- Some- Mostly Very i .. SD at all do not what decided what agree much agree agree do not agree agree """ agree Acceptance ofgeneral 1 10 26 39 19 1 l 3.74 1.00 violence Acceptance of 22 37 24 14 2 0 l 2.50 1.11 interpersonal violence Hostility 03 30 35 22 8 1 0 3.05 1.03 toward women Sex role 06 23 38 25 4 2 0 3.05 1.07 stereotypes ’ Not surprising with collegiate-level athletes, there was not a large amount of variance among responses on all three of the sport-orientation variables (see Table 8). The most variability was seen with respondents’ win orientation, while the least variability was seen with respondents’ goal orientation. No one indicated that they strongly disagreed with any of the statements on any of the three subscales, although a few people indicated some disagreement with some of the items. 79 Table 8 Sport Orientation Scale Percentages at Pre-Test l 1 r 2 3 4 5 Variable Strongly Slightly Neither Slightly Strongly agree agree agree nor disagree disagree disagree Competitiveness 88 l l l l 0 Win orientation 5 8 i 36 5 1 0 Goal orientation 79 l 20 0 1 0 To measure sexual aggression severity, items were sealed in accordance with the classification procedures outlined by Koss & Gaines (1993) in order to score a man according to the most severe level of sexual aggression he had perpetrated (4-point scale, 0 = sexual nonaggression, 4 = attempted or completed rape). A score of 0, representing sexual nonaggression, was assigned to the men who responded “never” to all of the SES items. This score was obtained from 18% of the current study’s sample (see Table 9). A score of 1 represented uninvited sexual advances (e. g. cat calls and wolf whistles to women) and was obtained by 56% of the sample. A score of 2, representing unwanted sexual contact (e. g. unwanted touching of a woman’s buttocks, breasts, or genital area), was obtained by 13% of the sample. A score of 3, representing sexual coercion, was obtained by men who answered yes to items indicating that they had attempted or completed unwanted sexual intercourse by using continual arguments and menacing verbal pressure (2%). Finally, men who answered yes to items indicating that they had attempted or achieved unwanted penetration by giving a woman more alcohol or drugs than she could handle, threatening bodily harm, using physical force, or overcoming her by a group of men (Koss & Gaines, 1993) received a score of 4, representing attempted 80 or completed rape. The score of 4 was obtained by 11% of the sample. The sample mean for this scale was 1.31, SD = 1.13. Table 9 Percent Sexual Aggression Severity at Pre-Test l 1 0 1 2 ‘ 3 4 Scale Scale Sexual , Uninvited Unwanted Sexual Attempted Mean SD non- ‘ sexual sexual coercion or aggression advances contact completed l rape %atPre-test 18 l 56 13 l 2 11 1.31 1.13 The scale mean for pre-test scores of rape myth acceptance was 2.55, SD = .76 (7- point scale; 1 = not at all agree, 7 = very much agree). Respondents were generally unsupportive of rape myths, although only 4% said that they did not at_all agree with rape myth supportive statements. Eighty-two percent of respondents said that they either mostly did not agree or somewhat did not agree with statements endorsing rape myths. Seven-percent stated that they were undecided, and only one percent somewhat agreed with the statements. No one stated that he mostly agreed or very much agreed with the statements. At pre-test, most participants reported a relatively low likelihood of sexually aggressing (scale mean = 1.15, SD = .47) (5-point scale; 1 = never, 5 = always). Eighty— eight percent of participants stated that they would never do any of the sexually aggressive behaviors listed, even if they thought no one would know. Over 9% of participants stated that although it would be rarely, they would be willing to force a woman into doing something sexual by using verbal coercion, alcohol, or physical force. 81 One percent stated that they would sometimes do any of the behaviors (score of 4). The remaining two percent stated that they were unsure whether they would do any of the sexually aggressive behaviors listed, and no one stated that he would always do any of the behaviors (see Table 11). Table 10 Percent Rape My1h Acceptance at Pre—Test 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Scale Scale Not Mostly Some- Un- Some- Mostly Very 1 Mean SD at all do not what do decided what agree much agree agree not agree agree agree % at Pre- 4 5 1 3 l 7 1 O 0 2-55 .76 test " Table 11 Percent Likelihood of Sexually Aggressing at Pre-Test 1 2 3 4 Never Rarely Unsure Sometimes % at 88 9 2 l Pre-test At pre-test, overall, participants reported a relatively high proclivity of confronting a person if they thought a woman was being forced to do something sexual that she did not want to do (see Table 12). Percentage of respondents clairrring they would interrupt the situation while it was happening ranged from 56% to 85% across a_ll scenarios. Percentage of respondents who stated that they would wait and talk with the 82 man ranged from 6% to 29% across all scenarios. The percentage of respondents who stated that they would wait to talk with the woman was slightly lower, ranging from 4% to 9% across all situations. A range of 3% to 29% of respondents claimed that they would do nothing. Participants were most likely to intervene when both the man and the woman in the scenario were known to the participant. For example, in the scenario in which a teammate is kissing a woman who is a good friend of the participant, 85% of respondents claimed that they would interrupt the situation. Likewise, in the scenario in which a teammate is making a woman do something sexual and she is a good fiiend of the participant, 82% of respondents stated that they would interrupt the situation. The above two scenarios were also the two scenarios in which participants were least likely to do nothing. Only 3% of respondents stated that they would do nothing if they knew both people in the situation. 83 Table 12 Percent of Responses by Item for Likelihood of Confronting at Pre-Test Do Wait & Wait & Interrupt nothing talk with talk with the woman man situation Sit. 1 See a teammate kissing a woman. . .do not 20% 4% 20% 56% know woman Sit. 2 Know a man is making a woman do 14% 9% 10% 68% something sexual. . . do not know man Sit. 3 Know a teammate is making a woman do 3% 5% 10% 82% something sexual. . . woman is good friend Sit. 4 See a teammate kissing a 3% 6% 6% 85% woman. . .woman is good friend Sit. 5 Know a teammate is making a woman do 18% 4% 19% 59% something sexual. . . do not know woman Sit. 6 See a man kissing a woman. . . do not know 29% 9% 6% 57% man Multivariate Analyses To test the hypotheses that involvement in this program would decrease men’s reports of their rape myth acceptance and likelihood of sexually aggressing and increase men’s reports of their likelihood of confronting a situation, repeated measures multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) was used to initially compare pre and post- test scores. Dependent variables for the MAN OVA were rape myth acceptance, likelihood of confronting, and likelihood of sexually aggressing. The independent variable was time (two time-points of pre-test and post-test). Total N = 120. 84 mm 7 2b.- #2. woo. onor mmo. 23v socficoto 53 E _. coo. ovo. moor _mo.- ovo. 3x409 couficoto .80 who. vwo. moo. So. So: woo. 3208 305253800 oo _ .- mm _ .- _m _. wvo. we _ .- MN o .- nZOUo cocaomfitam room 83:00-52 co 68:00 So: 5.. Ear who. o2. omor K L .- ago 38% BEwBBEQE 5 oofiamomtmm as»; u ......mm m 533. ... mmmr wwor ...o o N. 1. mvm. 95$ :o_mm8ww< 338m SFCO bigom ......owm 1&3. moor moor .....Nmm. tQmm. 305 mamboocoom 20m xom Iowv .....onm. mm _ .- mm? :53. zoom. 89$ 5an “US$81 bzcmom ......omm .....onm. Rot voor .....owm. 1.3 m. 90$ 85.05 36:00 mo 85383.. .583: .....omm. nmor mo_. .....oom. z. 5v. A33 8:205 1385985 we 853686. ooo; 55%. 1.30. cm _ .- 1. wmv. 2.94m. Ago “mom 3 8.5383. 532 comm ......ono ooo; .....oomr $03.. $an. 1.3%. 2325 8m “a 83383. :52 and 2.30. 2.80. ooo._ 1.3». 153.. we? Amud “mom 8 wEEomEoU mo 80:28:; om? .....ommr Imam. ooog wmo... mgr God 05 «a mad—Hoodoo «0 8228—5 ......wmv. 1.an ERR: wmor ooo._ Iowv. Am0m *var 3% o. ooo._ .2..me 2.3m. Immm. 30% wfinboocoom 20m xom mmor .....Nwm. .3..me ooo._ EL. 3.. 5w. 605 00603 @330..- 5:503 _ 5.- moor wamvm. 2:. ooo; 1. omv. 90$ 0000—05 18050 o0 0003303.. o _ o.- 3:. .....mmm. 5.. 5w. .....omv. ooo._ A>Hm 40m mom >O< >2 llllllllllllllllllllllllllllllllllllllllllllll ween—«.3. 30:02:32 E comb 833:3 m0 N232 00:29:00 2 035—. 86 325$ 35 8.0 2: a 285%: m: 8:20.80 .. 235$ .92 3.0 2: a 389%: 2 8:288 ...... 0002 .....m 3. 20$. 1.31m. 23v 5:00:35 53 3.0;. 000.: 1.03. m: _. 3 n mmo. N3. mi. .80 _ .- 95$ :0_m8:ww< 330m :0th m0 £85m ......womr 05.- moor :93... 305 wEaonEm 20m xom 30.. of. wofi ...mw _ .- $05 :0803 0330.2. @238: 1.03.- who. oofi- om _ .- 904$ 8:203 18:00 .20 85380.0. on 2 .- 03.. 32.- .....wbmr A23 8:203 3:028:85 no 853083: mm _ .- :2. mg. 00 2 .- @325 :8: :0 8:8:80< :32 03% mgr coo. 30. m m 2 .- 2325 05 :0 8:8080< :52 000% K 2. 0:0. moo. _m _. ANUAV 50m :0 w50:0¢:00 m0 2000:2025 wwo. 30.. .30. wvo. Cod 8m :0 wE::0¢:0U .«0 0005—025 08.- 28.- :o... wit Am 142 Sport Orientation Questionnaire Please circle the number of years you have participated as an intercollegiate athlete at MSU (including redshirt years): 1 2 3 4 5 more than 5 The following statements describe reactions to sport situations. We want to know how you usually feel about sports and competition. Read each statement and circle the letter that indicates how much you agree or disagree with each statement on the scale: A, B, C, D, E. There are no right or wrong answers; simply answer as you honestly feel. Do not spend too much time on any one statement. Remember, choose the letter that describes how you usually feel about sports and competition. Strongly Slightly Neither Slightly Strongly agree agree agree nor disagree disagree disagree 1. I am a determined competitor. A B C D E 2. Winning is important. A B C D E 3. I am a competitive person. A B C D E 4. I set goals for myself A B C D E when I compete. 5. I try my hardest to win. A B C D E 6. Scoring more points than my opponent is very important to me. A B C D E 7. I look forward to competing. A B C D E 8. I am most competitive when I try to achieve personal goals. A B C D E 9. I enjoy competing against others. A B C D E 10. I hate to lose. A B C D E 11. I thrive on competition. A B C D E 143 12. 13. 14. 15. 16. 17. 18. 19. 20. 21. 22. 23. 24. 25. I try hardest when I have a specific goal. E My goal is to be the best athlete possible. E The only time I am satisfied is when I win. E I want to be successful in sports. E Performing to the best of my ability is very important to me. E I work hard to be successful in sports. E Losing upsets me. E The best test of my ability is competing against others. E Reaching personal performance goals is very important to me. E I look forward to the opportunity to test my skills in competition. E I have the most fun when I win. E I perform my best when I am competing against an opponent. E The best way to determine my ability is to set a goal and try to reach it. E I want to be the best every time I compete. A B C D E Please turn to the next page :> 144 Sex Role Stereotyping Scale Please use the following scale to indicate how strongly you agree with each of these statements. Write the number of your choice on the line to the left of the statement. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Not at all Mostly do Somewhat Undecided Somewhat Mostly Very agree not agree do not agree agree much agree agree 1. A man should fight when the woman he’s with is insulted by another man. 2. It is acceptable for the woman to pay for the date. 3. A woman should be a virgin when she marries. 4. There is something wrong with a woman who doesn’t want to marry and raise a family. 5. A wife should never contradict her husband in public. 6. It is better for a woman to use her feminine charm to get what she wants rather than ask for it outright. 7. It is acceptable for a woman to have a career, but marriage and family should come first. 8. It looks worse for a woman to be drunk than for a man to be drunk. 9. There is nothing wrong with a woman going to a bar alone. Please turn to the next page :> 145 Sexual Experiences Survey Please indicate how frequently you have engaged in each of the listed behaviors. Please remember that all of your responses are strictly confidential. The response codes indicate: N = Never 1 = Once 2 = Twice S = Sometimes O = Often Since you were 14 years old 1. Made “cat-calls”, whistled, or yelled at a woman on the street. N 1 2 S O 2. Touched a woman’s buttocks, breasts, or genital area against her wishes. N 1 2 S O 3. Attempted sexual intercourse with a woman when she didn’t want to by overwhelming her with continual argument and pressure. N 1 2 S O 4. Attempted sexual intercourse with a woman when she didn’t want to by giving her more alcohol or drugs than she could handle. N 1 2 S O 5. Attempted sexual intercourse with a woman by threatening to use force. N 1 2 S O 6. Attempted sexual intercourse with a woman when she didn’t want to by using some degree of force. N 1 2 S O 7. Had sexual intercourse with a woman when she didn’t want to by overwhelming her with continual argument and pressure. N 1 2 S O 8. Had sexual intercourse with a woman when she didn’t want to by giving her more alcohol or drugs than she could handle. N 1 2 S O 9. Had sexual intercourse with a woman by threatening to use force. N 1 2 S O 10. Had sexual intercourse with a woman when she didn’t want to by using some degree of force. N 1 2 S O 146 1 I. Stood in line to have sex with a “party girl.” 12. Had sexual intercourse with a “groupie.” 13. Stood in line to have sex with a “groupie.” 14. Had sexual intercourse with a woman who protested, but who really wanted to have sex. 147 280 280 280 \ln Likelihood of Sexually Aggressing (italicized statements are filler items) Please use the following scale to determine your responses to the question below: 1 2 3 4 5 never rarely unsure sometimes always If you could be assured that no one would know, how often would you engage in each of the following behaviors: Group sex __ Sex with someone else besides your steady boy/girlfriend __ Oral sex __ Force a woman to do something sexual she doesn’t want to do __ Bondage _ Anal sex __ Talk a woman into doing something sexual she doesn’t want to do Same-sex sex Force a woman to have sex when she doesn’t want to Use alcohol in hopes of having a woman give in to something sexual she wouldn’t give in to if not drinking 148 Likelihood of Confronting People do different things under different circumstances. What do you think you would do in the following situations? Please write the number to the left of the situation: do nothing wait for a more convenient time to talk to the woman wait for a more convenient time to talk with the man interrupt the situation to ask if everything is alright none of the above — please write what you would do next to the situation Ul-F-UJNl—t I You see a teammate kissing a woman who is pushing him away, and the woman is someone you do not know. You know a man is making a woman do something sexual that she does not want to do, and the man is someone you do not know. You know a teammate is making a woman do something sexual that she does not want to do, and the woman is a good friend of yours. You see a teammate kissing a woman who is pushing him away and the woman is a good friend of yours. You know a teammate is making a woman do something sexual that she does not want to do, and the woman is someone you do not know. You see a man kissing a woman who is pushing him away and the man is someone you do not know. Please turn to the next page :> 149 Illinois Rape Myth Acceptance Scale — Short Form Please write on the blank to the left of each question the number from the scale below that corresponds to how strongly you agree with each of the statements given. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Not at all Mostly do Somewhat Undecided Somewhat Mostly Very agree not agree do not agree agree much agree agree 1. __ If a woman is raped while she is drunk, she is at least somewhat responsible for letting things get out of control. 2. _ Although most women wouldn’t admit it, they generally find being physically forced into sex a real “turn on.” DJ If a woman is willing to “make out” with a guy, then it’s no big deal if he goes a little further and has sex with her. 4. __ Many women secretly desire to be raped. 5. __ Most rapists are not caught by the police. 6. _ If a woman doesn’t physically fight back, you can’t really say that it was rape. 7. __ Men from nice middle-class homes almost never rape. 8. _ Rape accusations are often used as a way of getting back at men. 9. __ All women should have access to self-defense classes. 10. __ It is usually only women who dress suggestively who are raped. 11. __ If the rapist doesn’t have a weapon, you really can’t call it rape. 12. __ Rape is unlikely to happen in the woman’s own familiar neighborhood. l3. __ Women tend to exaggerate how much rape affects them. 14. __ A lot of women lead a man on and then they cry rape. 150 15. 16. 17. 18. 19. 20. It is preferable that a female police officer conduct the questioning when a woman reports a rape. A woman who “teases” men deserves anything that might happen. When women are raped, it’s often because the way they said “no” was not clear. Men don’t usually intend to force sex on a woman, but sometimes they get too sexually carried away. A woman who dresses in skimpy clothes should not be surprised if a man tries to force her to have sex. Rape happens when a man’s sex drive gets out of control. Please turn to the next page :> 151 Hostility Toward Women Scale Please use the following scale to indicate how strongly you agree with each of these statements. Write the number of your choice on the line to the left of the statement. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Not at all Mostly do Somewhat Undecided Somewhat Mostly Very agree not agree do not agree agree much agree agree 1. I feel that many times women flirt with men just to tease them or hurt them. 2. I believe that most women tell the truth. I usually find myself agreeing with women. U) 4. __ I think that most women would lie just to get ahead. 5. __ Generally, it is safer not to trust women. 6. __ When it comes down to it, a lot of women are deceitful. 7. _ I am easily angered by women. 8. __ I am sure I get a raw deal from the women in my life. 9. _ Sometimes women bother me just by being around. 10. __ Women are responsible for most of my troubles. Please turn to the next page :> 152 Attitudes Toward Violence Scale (randomized order of Interpersonal and General) Please use the following scale to indicate how strongly you agree with each of these statements. 1 Not at all agree 2. 6. 7. 8. 9. 10. 11. 12. 13. 14. 15. 16. Write the number of your choice on the line to the left of the statement. 2 3 4 5 6 7 Mostly do Somewhat Undecided Somewhat Mostly Very not agree do not agree agree much agree agree Violent crimes should be punished violently. The death penalty should be palt of every penal code. A child’s habitual disobedience should be punished physically. Any nation should be ready with a strong military at all times. Punishing children physically when they deserve it will make them responsible and mature adults. The manufacture of weapons is necessary. It is alright for a partner to hit the other if they are unfaithful. The government should send armed soldiers to control violent university riots. An adult should whip a child for breaking the law. It is alright for a partner to hit the other if they flirt with others. Our country has the right to protect its borders forcefully. Any prisoner deserves to be mistreated by other prisoners in jail. Giving mischievous children a quick slap is the best way to quickly end trouble. Children should be spanked for temper tantrums. It is alright for a partner to slap the other if insulted or ridiculed. Young children who refuse to obey should be whipped. 153 17. __ War is often necessary. 18. _ It is alright for a partner to slap the other’s face if challenged. 19. __ Our country should be aggressive with its military internationally. 20. _ Killing of civilians should be accepted as an unavoidable part of war. 154 Please turn to the next page :> Thank you for completing the questionnaire packet! Please put it back into the envelope you got it from and place the envelope at your seat until the end of the workshop. 155 APPENDIX B Post-test Questionnaire Consent Form Likelihood of Sexually Aggressing Likelihood of Confronting Illinois Rape Myth Acceptance Scale — Short Form Group Utilization Questions Thank You Form 156 1. -;--~.-.- untrue-“"0! 1.- --. nun-m. “— -. Similar to the packet you filled out before the workshop, this packet contains questions about your attitudes and opinions about a variety of issues. Remember, your responses are completely confidential. ,' Please DO NOT write your name on any of the sheets in this packet. Your participation in this survey is completely voluntary, and you may refuse to participate at any time. You may refuse to respond at any ; '_ time to particular questions without penalty. You indicate your 3 : voluntary agreement to participate by completing and returning this ’ questionnaire. You will have 10 minutes to complete this packet. .- Some of the questions are highly personal, and in the case that any of the topics presented upsets or offends you, a list of helpful phone numbers and resources has been included in your folder. You may also contact the project investigator (Wendi Siebold) if you have any questions or concerns about this survey at: 353-5015. When you have completed your packet, place it in the manila envelope with your other questionnaire packet and give the whole envelope to the people at the door. Remember, don’t put your name anywhere on the pages or the envelope! Thames again/t for heLle/tgl Please turn to the next page :> 157 Likelihood of Sexually Aggressing (italicized statements are filler items) Please use the following scale to determine your responses to the question below: 1 2 3 4 5 never rarely unsure sometimes always If you could be assured that no one would know, how often would you engage in each of the following behaviors: Group sex Sex with someone else besides your steady boy/girlfriend Oral sex _ Force a woman to do something sexual she doesn’t want to do __ Bondage Anal sex Talk a woman into doing something sexual she doesn’t want to do Same-sex sex Force a woman to have sex when she doesn’t want to Use alcohol in hopes of having a woman give in to something sexual she wouldn’t give in to if not drinking 158 , 1,11,. Likelihood of Confronting People do different things under different circumstances. What do you think you would do in the following situations? Please write the number to the left of the situation: do nothing wait for a more convenient time to talk to the woman wait for a more convenient time to talk with the man interrupt the situation to ask if everything is alright none of the above — please write what you would do next to the situation UI-[AUJNl—t I You see a teammate kissing a woman who is pushing him away, and the woman is someone you do not know. You know a man is making a woman do something sexual that she does not want to do, and the man is someone you do not know. You know a teammate is making a woman do something sexual that she does not want to do, and the woman is a good friend of yours. You see a teammate kissing a woman who is pushing him away and the woman is a good friend of yours. You know a teammate is making a woman do something sexual that she does not want to do, and the woman is someone you do not know. You see a man kissing a woman who is pushing him away and the man is someone you do not know. Please turn to the next page :> 159 Illinois Rape Myth Acceptance Scale - Short Form Please write on the blank to the left of each question the number from the scale below that corresponds to how strongly you agree with each of the statements given. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Not at all Mostly do Somewhat Undecided Somewhat Mostly Very agree not agree do not agree agree much agree agree 1. If a woman is raped while she is drunk, she is at least somewhat responsible for letting things get out of control. 2. Although most women wouldn’t admit it, they generally find being physically forced into sex a real “turn on.” 3. If a woman is willing to “make out” with a guy, then it’s no big deal if he goes a little further and has sex with her. 4. __ Many women secretly desire to be raped. 5. _ Most rapists are not caught by the police. 6. _ If a woman doesn’t physically fight back, you can’t really say that it was rape. 7. _ Men from nice middle-class homes almost never rape. 8. __ Rape accusations are often used as a way of getting back at men. 9. _ All women should have access to self-defense classes. 10. __ It is usually only women who dress suggestively who are raped. l 1. __ If the rapist doesn’t have a weapon, you really can’t call it rape. 12. __ Rape is unlikely to happen in the woman’s own familiar neighborhood. 13. _ Women tend to exaggerate how much rape affects them. 14. _ A lot of women lead a man on and then they cry rape. 15. _ It is preferable that a female police officer conduct the questioning when a woman reports a rape. 160 16. 17. 18. 19. 20. A woman who “teases” men deserves anything that might happen. When women are raped, it’s often because the way they said “no” was not clear. Men don’t usually intend to force sex on a woman, but sometimes they get too sexually carried away. A woman who dresses in skimpy clothes should not be surprised if a man tries to force her to have sex. Rape happens when a man’s sex drive gets out of control. Please turn to the next page :> 161 Utilization Questions What factors would influence your decision to confront a teammate who was making a woman do something sexual that she does not want to do? How much do you think the information you learned in this workshop would help you stand up to a teammate if he was doing something with a woman that you thought was wrong? (Please check one) Cl Won’t help me at all, in any situation Cl Might help me in certain situations Cl Will help me in most situations Cl Will help me in all situations Cl I already would have stood up to a teammate in any situation If you answered that it won’t help you, briefly explain why you don’t think it will help: How much do you think you will use some of the information that was presented in this workshop? Cl Very much CI Somewhat CI Not very much CI Not much at all What makes you say that? 162 How much do you think members of your team will use some of the information that was presented in this workshop? Cl Very much C] Somewhat Cl Not very much CI Not much at all What makes you say that? Please turn to the next page :> 163 Thank you {or participating lVL the worleshop todaw and filling out the questionnaires! Your answers will help l/tS determine how useful this worl’eshop is to studel/lts, and leal/l/l. what we CMVL do to Vidal/ea it more evg’ogable all/1.01 helpful to 501x. Please hand in your envelope with both of the questionnaires inside as you walk out the door. If 501A have Ell/La questions or concerns about MIA/5 of the questions 501A answered, please contact WCI/LOll Siebold at 353—5015. Thanks again! 164 APPENDIX C Table of Sexual Assault Prevention Programs Targeting College Students 165 Table 24 Sexual Assault Prevention Programs Targeting College Students”2 Notes and Key (Breitenbecher, 2000) Many authors report main effects for gender on the instruments used in sexual assault prevention studies. Effects for gender are not included in this table, unless such effects represent interactions between gender and experimental condition. In addition, effects described as “marginally significant” or “significant trends” are not included. In the sample column, numbers in parentheses indicate the number of participants who completed the follow-up phase(s) of the investigation, if this number was reported as differing from the original sample size. An equal sign (=) indicates nonsignificant differences between groups. B = experimental group. C = control group. Tx = treatment. Sig = statistically significant. AR = acquaintance rape. SR = stranger rape. Wk(s) = weeks(s). Mo(s) = month(s). Min = minutes. Hr(s) = hour(s). Pre = pretest. Post = posttest. F/u = follow-up assessment. DV(s) = dependent variable(s). Key to scale abbreviations is included at the end of the table Author(s) Sample Intervention(s) Evaluation Evaluation Design Results Berg, et al. 54 men E1: empathy Pretest, - LSA loaded on (1999) induction intervention single factor (audiotape of male 10 days later, - E1 > E2 and E2 > C survivor + factual 2-wk f/u (undesirable effect) info.) - AIV, ASB, IRMA, E2 = empathy RE, and ARE induction loaded on single (audiotape of factor. At f/u, E1 = female survivor + E2 = C factual info.) Borden et al. 100 men E = factual info., Pretest, 4-wk - ATR and RE: at f/u, (1988) and legal terms, f/u E = C women characteristics of rapists, rape trauma syndrome, prevention (45 min) C = no tx Dallager & 145 men E = human Pretest, - RMA: E < C Rosen (1993) and sexuality course posttest at - AIV: E = C women end of C = education semester course Ellis et al. 151 men Questions asking Question - RMA, MAR, ASB, (1992) and participants to either and AIV combined women consider a preceded (E) to form composite situation in which or followed score 166 a friend/relative disclosed that she had been assaulted (C) other measures - Emen>Cmen (undesirable effect) Fonow et al. 582 men Intervention was Pretest, 3-wk - RMA: at posttest, (1992) and either a live or posttest pretested groups women videotaped scored lower than program unpretested. Also, addressing myths, all experimental prevalence, rape as groups scored lower act of violence, than control groups. rape as community Live and video issue (25 min) interventions were E1 = pre, live, post equally effective. E2 = pre, video, RB, ASB, and SRS: post posttest means not C. = pre, post reported. E3 = live, post E4 = video, post C2 = post Foubert & 155 men E = Video in E1 = pretest, RMA: at post, E1 = McEwen which man being intervention, E2. Also, E1 = C. (1998) raped is described, immediate LSA: at post, E. = how to help posttest E2 = C. survivor, sexual E2 = SMCRP: was sig. communication, intervention, Negatively confront sexism immediate correlated with post and abuse of posttest RMA and LSA for women (60 min) C = posttest E1 and E2. C = no tx Gilbert at al. 75 (53) E = intervention Pretest, AIV, ASB, RMA, (1991) men based on ELM, intervention and SRS combined dramatic 1-2 wks later, into composite presentation, immediate change score: E > C negative posttest, 1- Phone call: E intrapsychic and social consequences associated with AIV, RMA, ASB, and male dominance ideology (60 min) C = no tx mo f/u phone call listened to more of call and made more favorable comments than C. No sig difference w/respeet to willingness to volunteer time NC: 1 of 3 items measuring state motivation, I of 2 items measuring ability, and 1 of 2 167 items measuring thought favorability were sig correlated w/change scores Gray et al. 70 E = personalized Pretest, BIDB: pre-post (1990) women intervention (e. g. intervention, change for E > C local statistics) immediate Perceptions of featuring role posttest vulnerability: pre- playing, myths, post change for E > dating behaviors, C only when and sexual married women communication excluded from C = sample nonpersonalized intervention featuring role playing, myths, dating behaviors, and sexual communication Hanson & 360 E = myths, videos Pretest, DB: at post, E < C Gidycz (346) of AR and intervention, SAA: at post, E < C (1993) women protective 9-wk f/u SC: at post, E = C behaviors, SV: at post, E < C prevention (60 for women without min) histories of assault C = no tx prior to intervention Heppner, 294 E1 = ELM-based Pretest, ELMQ: E] > E2 > C Humphrey et (258, intervention intervention TL: E1 > E2 > C al. ( 1995) 133) featuring 5-7 days RMA: only men and interactive, later, difference between women dramatic immediate groups was E2 men presentation of AR posttest, 5— < C men. E1 and E2 and protective wk f/u, 4- showed rebounding behaviors, sex role mos f/u, 5- at f/u. Rebound socialization (90 mos + lwk pattern for E1 = E2. min) f/u CCC: For men, E; > E2 = didactic, video of survivors (90 min) C = stress management E2 > C. For women, E1 = E2 = C. Willingness to volunteer, time thinking, time talking, and # people talked to: E > E2 and C. - Fee increase and 168 willingness to recommend: E1 = E2 = C. Heppner, 119 (57) E1 = “colorblind” Pretest, 1-wk - Factor analysis of Neville et al. men intervention posttest, 5- RMA, SIARA, SA, (1999) E2 = culturally mos posttest LSA, and SVAWS- specific for SV yielded 1 factor African American (rejection of rape) participants - Rejection of rape: C = not described ANOVA showed only sig effect was low high low (rebounding) pattern - Cluster analysis of flu data indicated 3 groups: deteriorating, rebounding, and improving. Participants in improving group were more likely to be in either E1 or E2 than C - ELMQ: African Americans in E2 > than other participants Katz (1995) male None None athletes Lee (1987) 24 men E = myths, E1 = pretest, - ATR-L: at post, E1 account of male intervention, = E2. Also, E1 survivor, guided immediate showed sig pre-post fantasy (imagine posttest change observing E2 = roommate intervention, engaging in immediate coercive sex) (2 posttest hI) Lenihan et al. 821 E = video of AR E1 = pretest, Paired t-tests: (1992) (445) effects on men and intervention - Women in E1 and men and women, several days C1 showed sig pre- women characteristics of later, posttest post change on rapists, cultural reasons, reasons 1 mos later C1 = pretest, ASB, SCNS, and AIV 169 why AR not posttest - Women in E1 only identified as rape, E2 = showed sig pre-post and female intervention, change on RMA survivor posttest 1 - Men in E1 and C1 describing effects mos later did not show sig on her (50 min) C2 = posttest pre-post changes on any scale ANOVA: - RMA: E1 and E2 < C1 and C2 - ASB, SCNS, and AIV: E1 and E2 = C1 and C2 - ASB: 2-way interaction between pretest and experimental condition (f/u analyses not reported) - AIV: 3-way interaction between gender, pretest, and experimental condition (f/u analyses not reported) Lonsway et 99 (92, E = semester long Precourse - IRMA: at post, E < al. (1998) 55) men course that trains assessment C. At f/u, E< C. and students to (E also - AHSB: at post, women facilitate rape repond to EC. C = semester long sexual At f/u, E = C. human sexuality conflict), - Responses to course postcourse videotaped sexual assessment conflict: E more (B also likely(than at respond to pretest) to use direct videotaped verbal resistance. E sexual less likely to use conflict), 2- indirect verbal yr f/u resistance, indirect physical resistance, and internal monitoring. No 170 change in use of direct physical resistance. Mann et al. 92 men E1 = dramatic Intervention, Post ( 1988) and presentation + immediate - SAS: E1 > E3 = C women discussion (30 posttest, 5- - SRAS: E1 = E2 = E3 min) wk f/u = C E2 = dramatic presentation (15 F/u min) - SAS: main effect E3 = discussion for condition sig (15 min) (pattern of means C = no tx not reported) - SAS: interaction between condition and previous SA, but authors warn about small cell sizes. Among those with low SA, E1 > E3 and C - SRAS.E1= E2 = E3 = C Parrot et al. male None None (1994) athletes Pinzone- 152 men E = AR prevention Pretest, - RE: E showed Glover et al. and program, myths, intervention greater pre-post (1998) women characteristics and 1 wk later, 1 change than C behaviors of wk f/u - RMA: at f/u E = C rapists, prevention - ATW: Among men, (60 min) pre-post change for C = sexually E > C. Among transmitted disease women, pre-post prevention change for E = C program — Responses to AR scenarios: At f/u, men in E were more likely to identify scenario as rape than men in C. At f/u, among women E = C. Ring & Men, E = destructive No formal Men reported greater Kilmartin number aspects of men’s evaluation willingness to (1992) not sex role was participate because of reported socialization, film, conducted non-threatening l71 experiential exercises to increase awareness of objectification of men’s and women’s bodies and media’s victimization of women nature of program, feeling less defensive, and greater awareness about destructive aspects of male socialization. Schaeffer & 160 men Specific program Quasi- - RMA: E = C Nelson not tested or experimental - ATW: E = C (1993) described. E = men who had previously attended a rape education program C = men who had not Schewe & 74 E1 = video of Pretest, - RCA: E1 = E2 = C O’Donohue “high- victim intervention, - AIV: E1 and E2 (1996) risk” empathy/outcome 2-wk f/u showed sig pre- men expectancy post changes program (50 min) - ASB: E2 showed E2 = video of rape sig pre-post supportive changes cognitions - RMA: E2 showed program (50 min) sig pre-post C = no tx changes - ASA: E1 and E2 showed sig pre- post changes. l Portions of this table and table format were adapted from Breitenbecher (2000). 2 This table includes only programs that were discussed within this paper, and is not exhaustive of all programs in the literature. For a full table of programs in the literature up to the year 2000, please refer to Breitenbecher (2000). 172 Key to scale abbreviations (reprinted from Breitenbecher, 2000) AHSB = Adversarial Heterosexual Sexual Beliefs Scale (Lonsway & Fitzgerald, 1995) AIV = Acceptance of Interpersonal Violence (Burt, 1980) ARE = Acquaintance Rape Empathy Scale (Berg etal., 1999) ASA = Attraction to Sexual Aggression Scale (Malamuth, 1989) ASB = Adversarial Sexual Beliefs (Burt, 1980) ATP = Attitudes Toward Feminism and the Women’s Movement Scale (Fassinger, 1994) ATR = Attitudes Toward Rape Scale (Field, 1978) ATR-L = Attitudes Toward Rape Scale (Lee, 1987) ATR-R = Attitudes Toward Rape Scale — Revised (Harrison et al., 1991) ATSI = Attitudes Toward Sexuality Inventory (Patton & Mannison, 1993) ATW = Attitudes Toward Women Scale (Spence et al., 1973) BIDB = Items assessing behavioral intent to avoid high-risk dating behaviors (Gray et al., 1990) CCC = Comprehension of Consent/Coercion Measure (Gibson & Humphrey, 1993) DB = Dating Behavior Survey (Hanson & Gidycz, 1993) DRAS = Date Rape Attitudes Survey (Holcomb etal., 1993) DSPARS = Dating Self-Protection Against Rape Scale (Moore & Waterman, 1999) ELMQ = Elaboration Likelihood Model Questionnaire (Heppner et al., 1995) FDR = Forcible Date Rape Scale (Giarusso, 1979) IRI = Interpersonal Reactivity Index (Davis, 1980) IRMA = Illinois Rape Myth Acceptance Scale (Payne et al., 1999) LSA = Likelihood of Sexual Aggression, including any modification or combination of items from the Likelihood to Rape Scale (Briere & Malamuth, 1983; Malamuth, 1981) or other items devised by the author(s) to measure this construct. MAR = Myths About Rape (Costin & Schwarz, 1987) NC = Need for Cognition Scale (Cacioppo & Petty, 1982) RB = Attribution of Rape Blame (Ward & Resick, 1979, cited in Resick & Jackson, 1981) RCA = Rape Conformity Assessment (Schewe & O’Donohue, 1993b) RE = Rape Empathy Scale (Deitz et al., 1982) RMA = Rape Myth Acceptance Scale (Burt, 1980) SA = Sexual Aggression, as measured by the Sexual Experiences Survey (Koss & Oros, 1982, Koss et al., 1987) SAA = Sexual Assault Awareness Survey (Hanson & Gidycz, 1993) SAKS = Sexual Assault Knowledge Survey (Breitenbecher & Scarce, 1999) SAS = Sexual Attitude Scale (Meuhlenhard & Felts, 1987) SC = Sexual Communication Survey (originally developed by Hanson & Gidycz, 1993, and later revised by Breitenbecher & Gidycz, 1998) SCNS = Sexual Conservatism (Burt, 1980) SIARA = Scale for the Identification of Acquaintance Rape Attitudes (Humphrey, 1996) SRAS = Simple Rathus Assertiveness Schedule (McCormick, 1986) SRF = Speaker Rating Form (Heppner etal., 1995, adapted from Barak and LaCrosse, 1975) SMCRP = State Measure of Central Route Processing developed by Gilbert et al., 1991, based on Petty & Cacioppo, 1986) SV = Sexual victimization, as measured by the Sexual Experiences Survey (Koss & Oros, 1982; Koss et al., 1987) SVAWS-SV = Sexual Violence Subscale of the Severity of Violence Against Women Scale (Marshall, 1992) TL = Thought Listening (Heppner, et al., 1995, adapted from Heppner et al., 1988) 173 to”. .: teams? 25:... ..