$35.... 5.5 : .. rs 1:? 1.; 2.1.1. 5 I in... 9.1“. tflst . x .37 1; .3315 0 .5. .rv. at ‘ r . . x . (10.19 V 4...}: J. i. ca .57.. 3.: .25. u .. 6.2. 2 a ‘ .Lm...‘ a .2: fivfi : . .vtn....:« : ‘- THESIS tool LIBRARY Michigan State University This is to certify that the thesis entitled PERCEPTION AND SUPPORT FOR CENSORSHIP OF INTERNET PORNOGRAPHY: THE INFLUENCE OF INDIVIDUALISM/COLLECTIVISM AND INTERNET SELF-EFFICACY ON THE THIRD-PERSON EFFECT presented by BYOUNGKWAN LEE has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for MA . d . COMMUNICATION egree 1n Major professor Date fi'IB'Ol 0-7639 MS U is an Affirmative Action/Equal Opportunity Institution PLACE IN RETURN BOX to remove this checkout from your record. To AVOID FINES return on or before date due. MAY BE RECALLED with earlier due date if requested. DATE DUE DATE DUE DATE DUE ‘07:? 1) 6720023 6/01 c1CIRC/Datepr65-pt15 —— PERCEPTION AND SUPPORT FOR CENSORSHIP OF INTERNET PORNOGRAPHY: THE INFLUENCE OF lNDIVIDUALISM/COLLECTIVISM AND INTERNET SELF- EFFICACY ON THE THIRD-PERSON EFFECT By Byoungkwan Lee AN ABSTRACT OF A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF ARTS Department of Communication 2001 Professor Ronald C. Tamborini ABSTRACT PERCEPTION AND SUPPORT FOR CENSORSHIP OF INTERNET PORNOGRAPHY: THE INFLUENCE OF INDIVIDUALISM/COLLECTIVISM AND INTERNET SELF- EFFICACY ON THIRD-PERSON EFFECT By Byoungkwan Lee This study examined the influence of individualism/collectivism and Internet self- efficacy on third-person perception in the context of Internet pornography and the relationship between this third-person perception and support for censorship as its behavioral consequence. Results of a survey of both US. and South Korean college students (N = 232) supported the existence of the third-person effect in the context of Internet pornography. Participants in this study perceived Internet pornography to have a greater negative effect on others than on themselves (third-person perception), and the difference was significantly associated with support for censorship. The findings also confirmed the influence of individualism/collectivism and Internet self-efficacy on the third-person perception. Finally, the results of path analysis suggested that the data were consistent with a causal model in which both individuals’ individualism/collectivism and Internet self-efficacy influence third-person perception, which then leads to support for censorship of Internet pornography. ACKNOWELEDGEMENT This thesis would not have been completed without assistance and encouragement of many people. I would like to be deeply thankful to Dr. Ron Tarnborini, my advisor, for his support, direction, patience, and encouragement throughout my master studies. I wish to express my deep gratitude to all my committee members, Dr. Hairong Li and Dr. Stacy Smith, for their valuable comments. I really appreciate Dr. Yong-sung Lee, Dr. Hyung-il Kim, and Mr. Ki-jung Lee for their help in collecting data. Finally, I thank all my family. Specially, I thank my wife, Joung-a, and my son, Min-young, who have been encouraging me. iii TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES .................................................................................... v LIST OF FIGURES .................................................................................. vi INTRODUCTION .................................................................................... 1 CHAPTER 1 LITERATURE REVIEW AND HYPOTHESES ................................................. 5 Internet Pornography ....................................................................... 5 Third-Person Perception and Support for Censorship of Media Content ............ 8 Hypotheses ................................................................................ 15 A Model of Culture and Internet Self-Efficacy’s Impact on the Third—Person Effect ............................................................. 20 CHAPTER 2 METHOD .................................................................................................................. . 22 Sample........ ......................................................................................................... 22 Procedures ............................................................................................................ 22 Measures .................................................................................................................. 23 CHAPTER 3 RESULTS ............................................................................................. 28 Third-Person Perception and Internet Pornography .................................. 28 Individualism/Collectivism and the Third-person Perception ....................... 29 Internet Self-Efficacy and Third-person Perception ................................. 29 Third-Person Perception and Behavior ................................................. 33 Culture and Internet Self-Efficacy’s Impact on the Third-Person Effect ............ 35 CHAPTER 4 DISCUSSION ....................................................................................... 37 APPENDIX .......................................................................................... 41 REFERENCES ....................................................................................... 59 iv LIST OF TABLES Table 1 — Modified Individualism/Collectivism (INDCOL) Items ......................... 24 Table 2 — Modified Internet Self-Efficacy Items ............................................. 25 Table 3 — Modified Support for Censorship on Internet Pornography ...................... 27 Table 4 — Self and Others Effects of Internet Pornography Comparison ................... 28 Table 5 — Pearson Correlation Matrix for Independent Variables and Dependent variables ................................................................... 30 Table 6 — Hierarchical Multiple Regression Predicting Third-Person Perception . . . . .32 Table 7 — Hierarchical Multiple Regression Predicting Support for Censorship of Internet Pornography ............................................................... 35 LIST OF FIGURES Figure l — Initial Path Model ...................................................................... 21 Figure 2 — Path Coefficients among Individualism/Collectivism, Internet Self- Efficacy, Third-person Perception, and Support for Censorship ............................ 36 vi INTRODUCTION The Internet is a double-edged sword that must be handled carefully. While useful information and a variety of entertainment are widely provided and consumed on the Internet, there has been a marked increase in sexually explicit material. Alluring text, chat rooms, bulletin boards, and newsgroups on the Internet have intensified parental anxiety about their vulnerable children. In fact, a simple search using the Yahoo or Alta Vista search engines reveals that there are hundreds of thousands of pornographic sites on the Internet. Moreover, “the absence of control, censorship, limits, or grading," as Weimann (2000) expressed, makes the Internet look like “an ideal site for pornography. " Censorship is seated at the heart of the controversy in the Internet pornography issue. A point of contention in this debate has focused on the First Amendment right to Internet content against the protection of minors from sexually explicit material on the Internet. In Reno v. ACLU (American Civil Liberties Union) (1997), there was a legal battle between the government and a coalition of business and flee speech advocates. The constitutionality of the Communication Decency Act of 1996 (CDA), which was passed by Congress to protect minors from sexually explicit material on the Internet, was taken to the Supreme Court. The Supreme Court ruled that the First Amendment right to Internet content must be upheld identifying “two Provisions of the CDA to be unconstitutional due to their vagueness and chilling effect their application would have on Internet communications” (Simon, 1998, p.1015). The Court’s decision meant that “any fiiture attempt to regulate sexually explicit Internet transmissions must be drafted with sufficient specificity such that no ambiguity exists as to the scope of its enforcement" (Simon, 1998, p. 101 5). In the fall of 1999, the Child Online Protection Act was before Congress. The gist of the act was to impose criminal penalties against commercial Web sites that display sexually explicit material on the Internet without appropriate measures to exclude minors (Munro, 1999). The constitutionality of this act is still pending a judicial decision, due to an appeal by the ACLU and the Electronic Privacy Information Center. The very core of this debate is the influence of sexually explicit material or pornography on individuals. The majority of research on pornography has empirically supported its harmful effects, at least on women and children, although the scientific community has not come to an absolute consensus about its attitudinal and behavioral effects (Beaver, 2000). Regardless of the lack of consensus about the harmful effects of pornography, public concern about Internet pornography has been increasing. Perhaps, people may think that sexually explicit material or pornography on the Internet is more harmful and dangerous than in traditional media, such as print, video, television, and film. This thinking might be attributed to individuals’ perceptual differences rather than the actual difference between the effects of Internet pornography and traditional pornography. The absence of regulation, the ease of access, and the plethora of pornographic images on the Internet may magnify people’s anxiety about the harmfirl influence of Internet pornography on minors. Consequently, this anxiety gives people the perception that Internet pornography is more harmful and dangerous than pornography in its traditional forms. This collective anxiety may affect public opinion about Internet pornography, influence decisions of policy makers, (Dearing & Rogers, 1996; Page & Shapiro, 1983) and shape judicial decisions related to the First Amendment right of free speech (Marshall, 1988; Mishler & Sheehan, 1993). The third-person effect hypothesis can provide a theoretical base to explain the relationship between public opinion about socially undesirable media content and their support of censorship for it. The third-person effect, as Greenberg and Salwen (1996) indicated, is rising "as a popular model generating a paradigmatic body of empirical research" (p. 76). Since the initial study of Davison (1983), which suggested that people will tend to overestimate the influence of mass communications on the attitudes and behavior of others rather than on themselves, a number of studies have examined the third-person effect hypothesis. Support for the third-person effect has been found in research examining the relationship between people's biased perception of socially undesirable media content’s influence and their overwhelming support of censorship in a variety of contexts including pornography and sexually explicit material (Gunther, 1995; Lee & Yang, 1996), television violence (Hoffner et. al., 1999; Rojas, Shah, & Faber, 1997), hate speech (Lambe & Shah, 1999), press coverage (Rucinski & Salmon, 1990; Salwen & Driscoll, 1997), misogynic rap lyrics (McLeod, Eveland, & Nathanson, 1997), advertising (Youn, Faber, & Shah, 2000), and general influence (Gunther & Hwa, 1996). Most research results in this area show that the individuals’ tendency to perceive greater media influence on others than on themselves (the perceptual component of third—person effect) leads to support for censorship of the media content (the behavioral component of third-person effect). The main purpose of this study is to extend research on the third-person effect to the context of Internet pornography. In addition, this study examines the influences of both individualism/collectivism and Internet use on third-person perception. Although the likely influence of culture might seem a critical factor in explaining individual differences in the third-person effect, the relationship between individualism/collectivism and third-person perception has not been empirically tested‘. Beginning from a cross-cultural perspective, this study examines whether cultural differences, specifically those distinguishing individualistic culture and collectivistic culture, affect third-person perception in the context of Internet pomography and, subsequently, leads to support for its censorship. This study also considers Internet use as a significant recipient factor that may affect third-person perception. Although literature in this area generally considers media use as an important recipient factor magnifying the third-person effect (Brosius & Engel, 1996; Chapin, 2000; Paxton, 1997; Price, Huang, & Tewksbury, 1997; Salwen, 1998; Salwen & Driscoll, 1997; Salwen & Dupagne, 1999), with the exception of only one study (Price et al., 1997) most research on traditional media surprisingly'fails to support the assumption that media use is a significant predictor of the third-person effect. However, we might expect the outcome to change when considering differences between Internet use and traditional media use. For example, while no special skill and knowledge are needed to watch television or read newspapers, these are required to access the Internet. As such, we might expect Internet use to influence the third—person perception more than is found with traditional media. 1 The research on other recipient factors has been well documented: age (Boynton & Wu, 1999; Tiedge et al., 1991), education (Driscoll & Salwen, 1997; Gunther, 1995; Salwen, 1998), gender (Boynton & Wu, 1999; Lee & Yang, 1996), and media use (Brosius & Engel, 1996; Chapin, 2000; Paxton, 1997; Price, Huang, & Tewksbury, 1997; Salwen & Dupagne, 1999). There has been only study (Park, 1998) on the relationship between cultural factor and third-person effect. Chapter 1 LITERATURE REVIEW AND HYPOTESES Internet Pornography By this time, numerous empirical studies have been conducted regarding exposure to sexually explicit material in terms of sexual arousal (e. g., Bancroft & Mathews, 1971), cognitive effects (e. g., Zillrnann & Bryant, 1982; Zillrnann, Bryant, & Huston, 1994; Zillrnann & Weaver, 1989) and behavioral effects (e.g., Donnerstein & Berkowitz, 1981; Zillrnann, Bryant, Comisky, & Medoff, 1981)2. However, very few of these studies focuses on Internet pornography. In addition, most of those studies focusing on Internet pornography have focused on the legal aspect of regulation (Cate, 1996; Simon, 1998; Vandiver, 2000) or content of Internet pornography (Mehta & Plaza, 1997; Rimm, 1995; Sanders, Deal, & Myers-Bowman, 1999). According to content analysis, pomographic images on the Intemet are not fundamentally different from those existing in magazines or videos OVIehta & Plaza, 1997; Sanders, Deal, & Myers-Bowman, 1999). As such, beyond anything resulting from the unique attributes of the Internet, we might expect strong third- person effects simply due to content. Nevertheless, attributes of Internet pornography could be a crucial deterrrrining factor in third-person effects. The popularity of Internet user and access, the incredible growth of the on-line sex industry, the global scale of dissemination, and the limits of control and regulation amplify public anxiety about the social impacts of Internet pornography. Some Internet use reports indicate this concern may be justified. According 2 Bryant and Zillmann (1996) broadly classified the effects of the consumption of sexually explicit materials or pornography into these three categories. to a recent report (Stanford Institute, 2000), over one third of the respondents3 regularly use the Internet more than five hours per day, with users who spend more than ten hours per day comprising a surprising 14 percent. A quarter of regular Internet users (more than 5 hours daily) feel that it has reduced their time with friends and family or attending events outside the home. In addition, many respondents indicated that their Internet usage has reduced both their television viewing and newspaper reading time. The economics of Internet pornography clearly demonstrate that the Internet has firmly entrenched itself in our society as a medium that transmits pornographic material. According to Slade (2000), about one third of the current commercial activity on the Internet is pornographic. He remarked: " Time says that adult Web sites collectively bring in nearly $1 billion a year, or about a tenth of entire sex industry, and predicts that the share will increase. The chief industry source also reports that pornography (loosely defined) accounts for 69 percent (or roughly $1 billion worth) of total Internet content sales. The same source maintains that about 84 percent of that 69 percent of content comes from American sources and sites. Large, hot sites can generate revenues of $1 million a month. Porn stars Asia Carrera, Jenteal, Mimi Miyagi, and Vanessa Del Rio have opened Web sites that can bring in up to $5 million a year, in some cases five times what they made from porn videos. Obviously these are rough figures, since if every site pulled in $5 million consistently then the aggregate numbers would be astronomical” (pp.238 - 239). From a practical point, it may be impossible to completely isolate minors from access to Internet pornography. Although all commercial Internet pomo-sites reqmre confirmation that the user is an adult, the confirmation screen itself displays pornographic images provided to attract and titillate new customers. In addition, non-commercial sites for Internet pornography are a crucial source for pornographic material, although there was a finding (Mehta & Plaza, 1997) that commercial vendors generally post more pornographic images than non-commercial distributors. Adolescents who cannot use credit cards for 3 The sample was of 4,1 13 adults in 2,689 households in the US. accessing commercial pomo—sites on the Internet share pornographic material with each other on the Usenet, chat rooms, personal sites, and so on.4 Although net filter software programs such as SurfWatch and Swaanny are employed to block youth access to adult pomo-sites, such net filters are not practical when they are used on public computers in schools and libraries because they often filter out more than they were intended to block (Melillo, 1999). Several Asian countries are prohibiting the production and dissemination of pomography or sexually explicit materials by law. South Korea, China, and Singapore consider Internet pornography as a serious threat to their social order and morals. In South Korea, for instance, there is no legal basis for punishing the dissemination of pornography through the Internet if the dissemination occurs outside the country. A recent instance in South Korea illustrates how Internet pornography becomes a serious social issue in countries that are prohibiting the production and dissemination of pornography by law. A video clip showing a top female pop star and a former producer having sex was uploaded on a commercial Internet pomo-site without her consent, and the video was posted on the personal web site of a college student who downloaded it by hacking. The video was shared with countless other users by using FTP and was disseminated at an amazing speed throughout the country. "For a short time, there was a cyber-war. Fans of singer Baek Ji- Young tried to paralyze Internet sites offering the video showing Baek and a former producer having sex. The Internet site built new firewalls in defense. In the end, the voracious nature of the Web could not be denied. The video clip multiplied and raced across the Internet, reportedly at a rate of 200,000 copies in one day. Back, 24, ’ Though at present there is no empirical evidence of this, we can easily observe this fact on any Usenet, chatting room, or personal site. See Gutfeld (1999) about pornography on chatting room and Mehta and Plaza (1997) about pornography on the Usenet. first tried to deny it was she. She hid on an island, then capitulated. She held a tearfirl news conference to confess and apologize... .. Since an estimated one-third of South Korea's 47 million people use the Internet - one of the highest rates in the world - 'it was like branding her with a scarlet letter,' quipped one observer” (The Washington Post, 2000). According to a report (Digital Chosun, 2000), the request for this video clip, throughout several search engines, placed it among the most frequent search requests for the year 2000. Internet pornography has proven itself sufficient to attract the concerns of public opinion and policy makers regardless of the debate of its actual effects. For them, public anxiety about Internet pornography may be more important than its actual impacts. In other words, people's perceptions that Internet pornography has harmful effects can influence policy makers and judicial rulings regulating pomography on the Internet. The third- person effect, as noted above, is a theoretical model to explain how people's perceptions about the effects of Internet pornography impact public opinion about regulating it. Third-Person Perception and Support for Censorship of Media Content The third-person effect hypothesis consists of two components, the perceptual component and the behavioral component. Gunther (1995) noted: "the first suggests that people are prone to a perceptual bias, leading them to estimate that a communication will have more influence on others than on themselves. The second component proposes that people may react in some way according to this estimate of larger effects on others” (p.27). In this study, the perceptual component of the third-person effect hypothesis refers to individuals’ tendency to perceive greater Internet pornography influence on others than themselves, and the behavioral component refers to individuals’ tendency to support censorship of Internet pornography on the basis of their perceptual discrepancy between perceived influence on self and others. The perceptual component. The first component of the third-person effect hypothesis is the perceptual discrepancy by which people are more likely to perceive greater media influence on others than on themselves. The perceptual discrepancy is the difference between the degree to which people estimate the effect of mass media on others and the degree to which people estimate that effect on themselves (Gunther, 1991, 1995). The hypothesis suggests that most individuals give higher estimates of media’s impact on the perceptions of others. The perceptual discrepancy can be explained in terms of biased optimism. Biased optimism is a tendency in which people believe that negative experiences occur to themselves less often than others (Weinstein, 1989). Exarrrining why the third-person phenomenon occurs, Gunther and Mundy (1993) suggested biased optimism as a cause of perceptual discrepancy’ . They insisted: "One explanation for this bias is ego—enhancement, that people are motivated to reinforce their self-esteem by seeing themselves as smarter or better than their peers. Such a self-serving bias offers a simple and persuasive explanation for the third—person phenomenon” (p.60). The optimistic bias phenomenon, however, depends on whether the type of message is harmful or beneficial (Gunther and Mundy, 5 Recently, on the contrary, a study (Chapin, 2000) did not support this assurrrption. This study empirically tested a relationship between optimistic bias and third-person perception from survey research with urban minority at-risk youth. The findings showed that although a weak relationship between optimistic bias and the third-person perception was found, the findings did not support their assumption that optimistic bias maybe an underlying mechanism of third-person perception. This result may be caused from the use of a unique sample presented as a limitation of the study. For more discussion, see Chapin (2000). 1993). In other words, while a harmful message should magnify the discrepancy between the estimated effect on the self and on others, a beneficial message should make no difference. This reversed third-person effect—that when people judge a message in communication as a positive or beneficial one, they perceive themselves to be more influenced than others would—has been verified also by many studies (Duck & Mullin, 1995; Duck, Hogg, & Terry, 1999; Duck, Terry, & Hogg, 1995; Eichholz, 1999; Gunther & Thorson, 1992; Henriksen & Flora, 1999). Findings consistent with the perceptual discrepancy hypothesis have been observed in several studies. Cohen and his colleagues (1988) supported claims that negative or undesirable messages in communication magnify perceptual discrepancy in the context of libel law. Conducting an experiment, they found that when the source of the defamatory communication is perceived as negatively biased, the discrepancy between perceived media influence on self and on others is greater. In the context of violent and misogynic rap lyrics, McLeod and colleagues (1997) also strongly supported the third-person perception hypothesis that the perceived effect of harmful messages on others would be greater than the perceived effect on self. Through a nationwide telephone survey about the O. J. Simpson trial, Salwen and Driscoll (1997) supported their third-person perception hypothesis that "people will perceive press coverage of the O. J. Simpson trial to exert a greater influence on other people's opinions than their own” (p.64). Boynton and Wu (1999) also supported the third-person perception hypothesis in research on TV violence. Respondents from a telephone survey showed a conspicuous perceptual discrepancy between the estimates of TV violence's influence on themselves and others. 10 In the context of pornography and sexually explicit material, two studies show strong support for the third-person perception hypothesis. Gunther (1995) conducted a telephone survey on respondents from the Continental US. age 18 or over (N = 648). He demonstrated clearly that people perceive pornography to wield greater influence on others than on themselves. The results showed that "a majority of respondents, 61%, perceived others to be more negatively influenced by X-rated material than themselves” (p.33). A study of Rojas and colleagues (1996) produced the same results. In a survey on college students (N = 133) at a midwestem university, they found that while only 7.1% of respondents perceived pornography to have a greater influence on themselves than others, 71.7% believed that pornography affects others more than themselves. Several studies attempted to examine the third-person effect hypothesis in a different cultural context. Lee and Yang (1996) examined the third-person perception hypothesis of sexually explicit television program in a Korean cultural setting. Since the dissemination of pornography is technically illegal in South Korea, the term "sexually explicit" used in this research is different from the term in the US. Thus, what is termed sexually explicit in their research referred to simple nudity, obscenity, and indecent sexual content. "Mild nudity" and "sexual violence" were used as the sexually explicit content in television programs in South Korea. Their findings in the Korean context replicated Gunther's observations (1995). Also, they found a gender difference in third- person perception. While males did not show a perceptual discrepancy between estimates of mediated sexual content's influence on themselves and same gender others, females tended to overestimate the influence of the mediated sexual content on same-gender others as compared to on themselves. Although this research has an implication in terms of 11 testing the third-person effect outside the US, differences in the effect across the two countries, US. and Korea, was not directly compared. Gunther and Hwa (1996) examined the third-person effect hypothesis in another Asian cultural context, Singapore. Data were collected from 506 randomly selected Singaporeans who evaluated ten categories of sensitive television content (violence, premarital sex, extramarital sex, foul language, illegitimate children, homosexuality, nudity, men with long hair, foreign programs, and religious programs). They found a perceptual discrepancy in all content categories. However, while respondents tended to perceive more negative influence on others than themselves when they evaluated the harmful influence of media content (c. g. sex-related categories, violence, and foul language), when they evaluated positive influence (e. g. religious programs), they estimated the content to wield more influence on themselves than others. In addition, they indicated the importance of cultural factors in the third-person effect research. They maintained: "People in this study exhibited the perceptual'bias just as strongly and consistently as have their counterparts in Western cultures where most third-person effect research has been conducted. At least in the non-Westem context of Singapore, the majority of people were very much inclined to separate their conceptions of self from that of others in questions of media influence” (Gunther and Hwa, 1996, p.262). Once again, however, this study did not provide empirical evidence on whether or not third-person effects differs between the two cultures. The belgvioral component. The behavioral component, as Salwen (1998) suggests, ' refers to "any behavioral outcome attributable to third-person perception" (p.262). As such, he suggests that people tend to act on the basis of these perceptual discrepancies. A direct 12 behavioral consequence of perceptual discrepancy appears as support for censorship of socially undesirable media content.6 Davison (1983) describes the phenomenon of censorship as one of the most interesting behavioral consequences of third-person perception of media content dealing with faith and morals. "Insofar as faith and morals are concerned, at least, it is difficult to find a censor who will admit to having been adversely affected by the information whose dissemination is to be prohibited. Even the censor's friends are usually safe from pollution. It is the general public that must be protected. Or else, it is youthful members of the general public, or those with impressionable minds” (p.14). Gunther first (1995) tested the relationship between third-person perception and support for the censorship of pomography as a behavioral consequence of the third- person effect. He raised a question, "is it the perception of greater negative influence on the morals of others that motivates people to support censorship of pornographic media content” (p.28). His research findings supported the behavioral hypothesis that the magnitude of perceptual discrepancy is positively related to support for censorship of pornography. Since then, research has left little doubt that a concomitant relationship exists between perceptual discrepancy and support for the censorship. Rojas and colleagues (1996) showed that as the perceptual discrepancy between media effects on others and self increases, people were more likely to exhibit pro-censorship attitudes toward three media topics, the media in general, violence on television, and pornography. 6 However, behavioral outcomes, according to Salwen and Driscoll (1997), may be affected by issue dynamics. Their study that examines the relationship between third-person perception and support for press restrictions on the O. J. Simpson trial coverage did not support the behavioral hypothesis. Rather, they found that people's predispositions about the trial interacted with perceptual bias as a predictor of support for press restrictions on trial coverage. They argued that issue legitimacy perceived as legitimate or illegitimate might influence people's willingness to support press restrictions. For more discussion, see Salwen and Driscoll (1997). 13 They also found a positive correlation between the magnitude of perceptual discrepancy and an intention to act in support of censorship for pornography (r = .44, p < .001). In the area of television film, Paxton (1996) showed that as the discrepancy between media effect on self and others increases, people are more likely to want to restrict the freedom of expression. Salwen (1998) and Salwen and Dupagne (1999) confirmed that third-person perception is a positive predictor of support for censorship of media content using hierarchical regressions. In the context of violent and misogynic rap lyrics McLeod and colleagues (1997) also tested the behavioral hypothesis and found a strong association between third-person perception and support for censorship of harmfirl content of rap music. Hoffner and colleagues (1999) examined the link between third-person effects and support for censorship of television violence. They measured and tested three different effects: mean- world perception, aggressive attitude, and aggressive behavior. The findings showed that while there was a strong association between the third-person effect for aggression and support for censorship, the third-person effect for a mean-world perception did not predict support for censorship. The behavioral hypothesis of the third-person effect has been supported also in Asian cultural contexts (Gunther & Hwa, 1996; Lee & Yang, 1996). Of particular importance to present concerns, Gunther and Hwa (1996) found that when people regard the media content as very sensitive, as with sex-related content, their support for censorship appeared to be stronger. However, of interest here is the fact that although two studies (Gunther & Hwa, 1996; Lee & Yang, 1996) have examined the third-person effect in an Asian cultural context and emphasized cultural factors as an important criteria for 14 explaining cultural differences in the third-person effect’s impact on censorship of socially undesirable media content, no true cross-cultural study on the third-person effect has been conducted. Hypotheses The purpose of this study is to extend research on the third-person effect to the context of Internet pornography and to consider the influence of two factors, cultural individualism/collectivism and Internet self-efficacy, on the third-person effect. For the purpose of this study, the following hypotheses are suggested. Third-person perception and Internet pomographv. The first hypothesis attempts to replicate previous findings on third-person perception in the context of Internet pornography. This study predicts the following: Hypothesis 1: Individuals will perceive Internet pornography to have a greater negative influence on others than on themselves. Individualism/collectivism and the third-person perception. As the studies of Gunther and Hwa (1996) and Lee and Yang (1996) intimated, it is strongly assumed that there are cultural differences in the third-person effect between Western culture and non- Westem culture. Gunther and Hwa (1996) postulated that the magnitude of the perceptual discrepancy in Asian cultures would be smaller than that in Western culture. In spite of their assumption, however, cultural differences in the third-person effect across Western culture and non-Westem cultures have not empirically tested. 15 In cross-cultural research, cultural variability is often considered along four dimensions: power distance, uncertainty avoidance, individualism/collectivism, and masculinity-femininity (Hofstede, 1991; Ting-Toomey, 1999). The present study focuses on individualism/collectivism. Individualism/collectivism, the most studied among the four dimensions of cultural variability, provides an important theoretical base for explaining "the basic differences and similarities concerning communication behavior between clusters of cultures" (Ting-Toorney, 1999, pp. 66—67). Individualism is defined as "the broad value tendencies of a culture in emphasizing the importance of individual identity over group identity, individual rights over group rights, and individual needs over group needs,” while collectivism refers to "the broad value tendencies of a culture in emphasizing the importance of the ‘we’ identity over the ‘1’ identity, group rights over individual rights, and in-group-oriented needs over individual wants and desires” (Ting-Toomey, 1999, p. 67). How individuals define themselves, according to Ting-Toomey (1999), is a fundamental base for making differences between individualism and collectivism across a wide variety of cultures. For instance, while individuals in individualistic cultures view themselves as independent fiom others and groups, individuals in collectivistic cultures view themselves as interdependent with others and groups. The degree to which individuals define themselves as independent or interdependent to others is termed as independent self-construal and interdependent self-construal (Markus & Kitayama, 1991). In terms of self-conception, according to Markus and Kitayama (1991), while individuals who have high independent self-construal tend to view themselves as distinct and unique from others, individuals who have high interdependent self-construal tend to fit in with 16 others. By comparing two cultural groups (Japanese and the US. college students), they found differences between two nations in their estimations of their own uniqueness. Thus, while the US. students estimated that only 30% of people on average would be better than themselves, Japanese students did not show this high self-perceived uniqueness (Markus & Kitayama, 1991). In general, while independent self-construal prevails in individualistic cultures, interdependent self-construal prevails in collectivistic cultures (Gudykunst et al., 1996; Markus & Kitayama, 1991; Singelis & Brown, 1995). Thus, according to- the findings of Singelis and Brown (1995), the more people tend to show collectivism, the more they show interdependent self-construal rather than independent self-construal. In this manner, it might be assumed that differences between the self-conception of individualistic cultures and collectivistic cultures create aggregate differences in cross-cultural optimistic bias. In other words, people in Asian cultures (collectivistic culture) are less likely to view themselves as distinct or unique from others and as smarter or better than others because they tend to show interdependent self-construal more than do people in Western cultures (individualistic culture). Research by Park (1998) partly supports this assumption. By comparing two cultural groups (Korean and the US. college students), she examined the relationship between self-construal and false uniqueness tendency. Her finding showed that the more individuals hold independent self-construal, the more they showed false uniqueness tendency. Since optimistic bias is thought to determine the magnitude of third-person perception, presumably, the expected cultural differences in optimistic bias should lead to difference between the two cultures in third-person perception. Therefore, this study 17 predicts that people who are high on collectivism are less likely to show third-person perception in the context of Internet pornography than are those who are high on individualism. Hypothesis 2: Collectivism will be negatively associated with third-person perception in the context of Internet pornography. Thus, the more individuals show collectivism, the more the perceptual discrepancy between the negative effects of Internet pornography on others and on themselves will decrease. Internet usL and third-person perception. As noted above, media use has been considered as an important recipient factor even though most studies have failed to support this assumption. However, this study assumes that the unique attributes of Internet use distinguish it from other media and magnify its impact on third-person perception. Central here are technological differences between Internet use and traditional media use. In order to use the Internet, special skills and knowledge are required. Eastin and LaRose (2000) argue: "The Internet requires development of a further set of skills that, to the novice user, at least, may be daunting. These include establishing and maintaining a stable Internet connection, learning how to navigate on the Internet, and searching it for relevant information”. Self-evaluation of one's capabilities to use the Internet differs. Some people may highly evaluate their capabilities to use the Internet, while others do not. This individuals' belief "in one's capabilities to organize and execute courses of Internet actions required to produce given attainments" is termed Internet self-eflicacy (Eastin & LaRose, 2000). They maintained: "Within social cognitive theory (Bandura, 1982; 1997) self-efficacy is a form of self-evaluation that influences decisions about what behaviors to undertake, the 18 amount of effort and persistence put forth when faced with obstacles, and finally, the mastery of the behavior. Self-efficacy is not a measure of skill; rather, it reflects what individuals believe they can do with the skills they possess” (Eastin and LaRose, 2000). It is important to note that Internet self-efficacy does not refer to the individuals' skills for using the Internet but rather to the beliefs of what he or she can do with the skills. This belief, namely, individuals' self-evaluation of capability to use the Internet, can also create optimistic bias. As noted above, optimistic bias is a tendency in which people believe that negative experiences occur to themselves less often than others. Optimistic bias can be influenced by perceived controllability (Brosius & Engel, 1996). Weinstein (1980) found that if people believed that they could control a negative event by their own effort, their estimate that likelihood that the negative event could happen to others is bigger than the likelihood it could happen to self. In a similar manner, this study assumes that Internet self-efficacy will influence third-person perception. Individuals whose self-evaluation of their own Internet use capability is high may view themselves more capable to control its influence. As such, they are likely to consider themselves less vulnerable to Internet pornography than others. Based on this logic, this study predicts that people who have high Intemet self- efficacy are more likely to show the third-person perception of Internet pornography than people who have low Internet self-efficacy. Hypothesis 3: There will be a positive relationship between Internet self- efficacy and third-person perception in the context of Internet pornography. Thus, the more individuals show Internet self-efficacy, the more the perceptual discrepancy between the negative effects of Internet pornography on others and on themselves will increase. 19 The behavioral component of the third-person effect. The final hypothesis is to replicate the behavioral consequence of third-person perception in the context of Internet pornography. This study predicts the following: Hypothesis 4: The magnitude of perceptual discrepancy will be positively associated with support for censorship of Internet pornography. A Model of Culture and Internet Self-Efficacy’s Impact on the Third-Person Effect Based on literature and the logic supporting hypotheses discussed above, this study constructed an initial path model to investigate the causal links relating individualism/ collectivism, Internet self-efficacy, third-person perception and its behavioral consequence (Figure 1). The flow of the model demonstrates sets of theoretical propositions regarding the interrelatedness of the factors. For instance, both individualism/collectivism and hitemet self-efficacy are shown as antecedents to third—person perception. Collectivism is a negative predictor of perceptual discrepancy, while self-efficacy is a positive predictor. Subsequent to this, third-person perception is seen as a positive predictor of its behavioral consequence, support for censorship of Internet pornography. 20 Figure 1: Initial Path Model Collectivism Support for Censorship Third- ers __, p on Internet pornography Perception mmmm Self-Efficacy 21 Chapter 2 METHOD Sample During May through June 2001 , a self-administered survey method was conducted using a convenience sample of college undergraduate students in communication courses from two nations, the United States and South Korea. These groups were selected for previously identified differences in cultural individualism/collectivism. The US. is the most representative individualistic country, whereas South Korea represents the collectivistic cultural area. According to Hofstede (1991) individualism index (IDV) values for 50 countries and 3 regions, the US. showed the highest IDV score (91), and South Korea ranked at 43rd (IDV score: 18) among 50 countries. The final sample size was 232 with 118 undergraduate students at Michigan State University in the US. and 114 undergraduate students at Hanyang University and Hanseo University in South Korea. Of participants, 47 percent were male, 53 percent were female, and the mean age was 21.44 (SD = 2.19, maximum = 34, minimum = 18). The mean age observed in the two groups did not significantly differ, t(230) = -.67, p = .95 (M age = 21.45 and 21.43 for the US. and South Korean students, respectively). Procedures Translation procedures in this study followed Hui's translation method. Two versions of each questionnaire (English and Korean) were created simultaneously. This method enables two versions of the questionnaire to have "equal status as being original" 22 (Hui, 1988, p.21). Three bilinguals checked the two versions of the questionnaire to assure the equivalence of the wording in different languages (Hui, 1988). The respondents answered questions about their cultural orientation, their Internet use and Internet self- efficacy, their opinion about the effects of Internet pornography and its censorship, and Socio—demographic information. Measures Individualism/Collectivism. Cultural individualism/collectivism is the construct measured by the individual's level of collectivism. In order to measure this construct, a modified version of Hui's (1988) individualism/collectivism (INDCOL) scale was employed. Hui reports a consistent pattern of construct validation. The INDCOL consists of six subscales: spouse, parent, kin, neighbor, fiiends, and co-workers (or classmates). The scale was modified through three steps. First, all items with an item-total correlation in subscales below .i20 were omitted. Also, three items (1 and 16 in the parent subscale and 11 in the co-worker subscale) were eliminated because they were redundant or do not translate well across the two cultures. For example, the proverb cited in item 11 of the co- worker subscale, "Too many cooks spoil the broth", has a different meaning for the collectivistic culture in South Korea. Second, a total 45 of items fiom Hui's 63 items were assessed for internal consistency and factor loadings. In order to perform these tests, Confirmatory Factor Analysis (CFA) was conducted using program PACKAGE (Hunter & Gerbing, 1982). Based on these tests, a total of 12 items that did not violate the internal consistency theorem were selected (see Table 1). 23 This 12-item modified scale showed unidimensionality with a low, but acceptable, internal reliability (or = .50). As such, the 12 items were summed into a single score for the analysis. Each item was measured on a 5-point Likert-type scale affixed by "strongly agree" and "strongly disagree." Higher scores on the summed score represented greater collectivism (M = 38.97, SD = 4.56, max = 51.00, rrrin =26.00). The mean for the US. participants was 37.58 (SD = 4.27), and the mean for Korean participants was 40.33 (SD = 4.42). The two groups significantly differed, t (223) = 4.75, p < .001. TABLE 1: Modified IndividualismIColIectivism (INDCOL) Items 1. If a husband is a sports fan, a wife should also cultivate an interest in sports. If the husband is a stock broker, the wife should also be aware of the current market situation.* When making important decisions, I seldom consider the positive and negative effects my decisions have on my parents. Teenagers should listen to their parents' advice on dating.* Whether one spends an income extravagantly or stingily is of no concern to one's relatives (cousins, uncles). When deciding what kind of education to have, I would pay absolutely no attention to my uncles' or cousins' advice. Each family has its own problems unique to itself. It does not help to tell relatives about one's problems. 7. I am often influenced by the moods of my neighbors.* 8. I enjoy meeting and talking to my neighbors everydayf 9. My good friends and I agree on the best places to shop.* 10. I would pay absolutely no attention to my close friends' views when deciding what kind of work to do. 11.There is everything to gain and nothing to lose for classmates to group themselves for study and discussion.* 12.I would help if a classmate at school told me that he/she needed money to pay something.* N 5".“ s» .01 * represents reverse scored items. 24 Internet Use and Internet Self-Efficag3y. A single item was used to measure Internet use. Respondents were asked an open-ended question on how many hours a week they use the Internet regardless of personal or professional use (open-ended questions). The mean on Internet use was 10.52 hours a week (SD = 11.47, max = 70.00, min =1.00). The construct of individuals' lntemet self-efficacy was measured with the 8 item Internet Self-efficacy scale developed by Eastin and LaRose (2000). After an internal consistency test using CFA seven of the eight items (see Table 2) showed good reliability (or = .87) and were summed into single Internet self-efficacy score for the analysis. Each item was measured on a 5-point Likert-type scale from 1 (strongly agree) to 5 (strongly disagree). All items were reverse scored. Higher scores on the summed score indicated greater Internet Self-efficacy (M = 21.48, SD = 5.34, max = 35.00, min = 7.00). TABLE 2: Modified Internet Self-Efficacy Items I feel confident understanding terms/words relating to lntemet hardware. I feel confident describing functions of I ntemet hardware. I feel confident trouble shooting Internet problems. I feel confident explaining why a task will not run on the lntemet. I feel confident using the lntemet to gather data. I feel confident Ieaming advanced skills within a specific lntemet program. I feel confident turning to on-line discussion group when help is needed. NP’P‘PP’NT‘ Third-Person Perception. Third-person perception was measured with four items adapted from Gunther (1995). The differences between perceived first-person and third- person effects of lntemet pornography both on moral values concerning sex and on attitudes toward the opposite sex were calculated to measure third-person perception. The 25 first two items measured the first-person effect, the estimated effects of lntemet pornography on self. The second two items assessed the perceived effects of lntemet pornography on others. Respondents were asked twice to indicate how they strongly agreed or disagreed with the same two statements asked in regard to themselves or others: 1) The pornographic content on the lntemet has a negative effect on your (other people's) moral values concerning sex and 2) The pornographic content on the Internet has a negative effect on your (other people's) attitudes toward the opposite sex. These questions were measured on a 7-point Likert-type scale from 1 (strongly agree) to 7 (strongly disagree). In this study the question order of self and others was not considered. Previous research has found that self-other question order does not influence the perceptual hypothesis of the third-person effect (Dupagne et al., 1999; Gunther, 1995; Salwen, 1998; Salwen & Driscoll, 1997) even though placement of self-other questions prior to the restrictions question might affect the behavioral hypothesis in some conditions (Dupagne et al., 1999). Support for Censors_hip_. In order to measure support for censorship on lntemet pornography, a modified version of Rojas et al.'s (1997) four pornography items (a = .81) extracted from 16 censorship items was employed. Respondents were asked to respond how they strongly agreed or disagreed with four statements (see Table 3). The result of CF A on the four-items showed unidimensionality (or = .72), and they were summed into single support-for—censorship score for analysis. These questions were also measured on 7-point Likert-type scales from 1 (strongly agree) to 7 (strongly disagree). Higher score 26 on the summed scores indicated greater support for censorship (M = 16.97, SD = 5.27, max = 4.00, min = 28.00). TABLE 3: Modified Support for Censorship on lntemet Pornography Items Pornography sites on the lntemet should be censored.* Today's standards for regulating pornography site on the lntemet are too strict. For lntemet pomographers freedom of expression should be restricted.* If pornography site on the lntemet has an appropriate device to block the access of children, it should not be restricted. swwe * represents reverse scored items. 27 Chapter 3 RESULTS Third-Person Perception and Internet Pornography Hypothesis I attempted to replicate previous findings on third-person perception in the context of lntemet pornography. The results supported the hypothesis 1. Individuals are likely to perceive lntemet pornography to have a greater negative effect on others than on themselves. The data show that 45.7 percent of participants estimated a greater negative effect of Internet pornography on others than on themselves, while 38.4 percent responded no difference in influence, and 15.9 percent evaluated more effect on themselves than on others. Paired t-test findings show that there are significant differences between the estimated effects of lntemet pornography on self and other both for moral values and for attitudes toward the opposite sex (Table 4). When responses on moral value and attitudes were combined, the perceptual discrepancy for scores combined was also significant. TABLE 4: Self and Others Effects of lntemet Pornography Comparison Self Others Perceptual N M SD M SD Discrepancy T-Value Effect on Moral Value 232 4.02 1.81 4.54 1.54 0.53 512*“ Effect on Attitudes 232 3.66 1.81 4.47 1.57 0.81 8.85*** Combined Effect 232 3.84 1.67 4.51 1.49 0.67 7.76*** **" p < .001. 28 Individualism/Collectivism and the Third-Person Perception Hypothesis 2 proposed that cultural individualism/collectivism influences third- person perception. Thus, people who are high on collectivism are less likely to show third-person perception in the context of lntemet pornography than those are low on collectivism. In order to test the association between individualism/collectivism and third- person perception, this study assessed the correlation between the summed INDCOL scores and combined perceptual discrepancy scores (i.e. effect on self versus others when combined for both moral values and attitudes: M = .67, SD = 1.31, max = 6.00, min = - 2.50). As hypothesized, Table 5 indicates that the summed INDCOL score was associated significantly with the magnitude of perceptual discrepancy (r = -.15, p < .05, one-tailed, n = 225). This finding supported the prediction of hypothesis 2 that individual’s collectivism is negatively associated with third-person perception. Thus, as an individual’s collectivism increases, the perceived difference between the negative effects of lntemet pornography on others and on themselves decreases. lntemet Self-Efficacy and Third-Person Perception Hypothesis 3 predicted that people who have high lntemet self-efficacy are more likely to show third-person perception of lntemet pornography than people who have low lntemet self—efficacy. To test this prediction, the summed lntemet self—efficacy scores were correlated with the combined perceptual discrepancy scores. The results support hypothesis 3. As Table 5 shows, there was a positive association between Internet self- efficacy and the magnitude of perceptual discrepancy (r = .13, p < .05, one-tailed, n = 227). Thus, as an individuals’ lntemet self-efficacy increases, the perceived difference 29 .5. Va... .8. v u. .toqaam .285 3629: 908 8:91 a Soto .985 pozmofig 98m 5:91 a .95: n F .oEom n c we cocaooom a .29: u _. SEES u c an penaooom m MO. ecNV. aim”. NOV 00. tsQNf $6... «5.3... «aw—fir UQ_£W._OWC@0 50% tOQQDW O.—. ram. ram..- .2. .9: 8. 8.- no... 8.- 388985 .5829. .m too. No. mo. «LN: vor tom... «of- 029:0 co Sootw .w met :3. tmmr No. .3... KC. 1.0m co mflootm .x. nor tum. :3. mo. mo. Ocean—meg fiEoE. .o nor 3. L r. no... .5002. .m «or tow. :FN. EEQEQOEOA “9:95 9 22890 .w 8. L F. om: “059:. .m tom. .xmm .N mm< .F m m n m m w m N F point: «copcaon new moi—3...; Eon—Boon:— Lo. x522 scam—230 coma—wed ”m m._m<._. 30 between the negative effects of lntemet pornography on others and on themselves increases. Though not a formal hypothesis here, the results of this study are also consistent with previous findings (Eastin & LaRose, 2000) in which Internet self-efficacy is positively correlated with lntemet use (r = .16, p < .01, one-tailed, n = 226). In addition, a positive relationship between lntemet self-efficacy and exposure to lntemet pornography was found (r = .24, p < .01, one-tailed, n = 226). Tests for modifying variables. For exploratory purposes, this study also conducted a hierarchical multiple regression to see if the observed associations hold when other independent variables are controlled, and to see if there are additional variables that could explain third-person perception. Six variables grouped into four blocks were entered into a multiple regression model (see Table 6) used to predict combined perceptual discrepancy scores. The overall model accounted for 5 percent of the variance (p = .06). The first block of the regression equation consisted of demographic variables (age and sex). The results show no significant effect for block 1. Although the two variables in this block were not significant, their patterns are consistent with previous findings. Past work has shown that age is negatively correlated with third-person perception (e. g. Boynton & Wu, 1999; Peiser, W. & Peter, J ., 2001; Salwen, 1998), and females tend to show more third-person perception than males (e. g. Boynton & Wu, 1999; Lee & Yang, 1996; Salwen, 1998). The second block consisted of media use variables (lntemet use and exposure to lntemet pornography). Neither was significantly related to third-person perception. The 31 third block consisted of lntemet self-efficacy. This block, although not significant, explained additional 2 percent of variance in the model. The standardized B of .13 for the final equation is relatively high within the model, and leaves open the possibility that Internet self-efficacy can be a notable predictor for third-person perception. The fourth block was INDCOL. As Table 6 demonstrates, INDCOL had a significant negative association with third-person perception (B = -.15, p < .05) and explained 2 percent of variance in the model. This suggests that collectivism remained a significant negative predictor for third-person perception even when controlling for other independent variables. TABLE 6: Hierarchical MuItIple Regression Predicting Third-Person Perception "‘ Final [3 R2 Change Block 1 .00 Age -.02 Sex b -.08 Block 2 .01 Internet Use -.07 Exposure to lntemet Pornography d .05 Block 3 .02 lntemet Self-Efficacy ° .13 Block 4 .02* INDCOL" -.15* Total R2 .05 a High score indicates greater third-person perceptual bias. b Recorded as 0 = female, 1 = male. ° High score indicates greater lntemet self-efficacy. d Recorded as 0 = some, 1 = none. ° High score indicates greater collectivism. * p < .05. 32 Third-Person Perception and Behavior Hypothesis 4 attempted to test the behavioral consequence of third-person perception in the context of lntemet pornography. This study predicted that the magnitude of perceptual discrepancy would be positively associated with support for censorship of Internet pornography. In most studies on the behavioral consequence of third-person perception, the independent influence of effect-on-self on the association between perceptual discrepancy and behavioral outcome has been controlled as a possible suppressor variable (see Gunther, 1995 or Salwen, 1998). Agresti and Finlay (1997) suggest that when no bivariate association is evident, if a third variable is related to both explanatory and response variables, it should be statistically controlled. Inspection of these data shows that effect-on-self is related both perceptual discrepancy (r = -.52, p < .01, one-tailed, n = 232) and support for censorship (r = .35, p < .01, one-tailed, n = 231). Thus, the suppressor effect of perceptual discrepancy on support for censorship was controlled in this study. First-order partial correlation between perceptual discrepancy and support for censorship controlling for effect on self shows that they are significantly and positively associated with each other (r = .27, p < .001, one-tailed, n = 228). This result supports hypothesis 4 that the more individuals perceive lntemet pornography to have a greater negative effect on others than on themselves, the more they are likely to support for censorship of lntemet pornography. This study affirmed the previous findings about the behavioral consequence of third-person perception (Gunther, 1995; Gunther & Hwa, 1996; Hoffirer et. al., 1999; Lee & Yang, 1996; McLeod et. al., 1997; Rojas et. Al., 1996; Salwen, 1998; Salwen & Dupagne, 1999). 33 Tests for modifyinuariables. In order to assess this first-order association and other factors in predicting support for censorship, a hierarchical multiple regression analysis was conducted. Eight variables grouped into three blocks were entered into the multiple regression model. The first block of the regression equation consisted of demographic and psychographic variables (age, sex, and INDCOL). Table 7 indicates that the first block explained 19 percent of the variance in the model (p < .001). Sex was a substantial and significant negative predictor of support for censorship (B = -.33, p < .001). The direction of age, although not significant, was negative ([3 = -.01, n.s.); again consistent with previous findings (e. g. Boynton & Wu, 1999; Gunther, 1995; Lee & Yang, 1996; McLeod, Eveland, & Nathanson, 1997; Rojas, Shah, & Faber, 1997; Salwen, 1998; Salwen & Driscoll, 1997). The second block consisted of media use and efficacy variables (Internet use, lntemet self—efficacy, and exposure to lntemet pornography). This block was not significantly related to the support for censorship. The third block consisted of perceptual discrepancy and perceived effect on self. This block explained 13 percent of variance in the model (p < .001). As Table 7 indicates, both perceptual bias and perceived effect on self were significantly associated with support for censorship (B = .25, p < .001 and B = .44, p < .001, respectively). These results strongly support hypothesis 4 that the magnitude of perceptual discrepancy would be significantly associate with support for censorship. 34 TABLE 7: Hierarchical Multiple Regression Predicting Support for Censorship of lntemet Pornography ’ Final [5 R2 Change Block 1 .19*** Age -.01 Sex b -.33*** INDCOL ° .00 Block 2 .02 Internet Use -.1 1 lntemet Self-Efficacy d .07 Exposure to lntemet Pornography ‘3 -.05 Block 3 .13*** Perceptual Discrepancy ' .25*** Self Effects ‘ .44*** Total R2 .33“:9 a High score indicates greater support for censorship. b Recorded as 0 = female, 1 = male. ° High score indicates greater collectivism. 6 High score indicates greater lntemet self-efficacy. ° Recorded as 0 = some, 1 = none. ' High score indicates greater effect. 9 The total R2 does not equal the sum of the R2 increment because of rounding error *” p < .001. Culture and Internet Self-Efficacy’s Impact on the Third-Person Effect Following these initial analyses, the proposed path model was tested to investigate the causal links among third-person perception, support for censorship, and two recipient factors, cultural individualism/collectivism and lntemet self-efficacy. To test the model, path analysis was conducted using PACKAGE (Hunter and Gerbing, 1982). The results of analysis for the missing path links between collectivism and support for censorship (error = .02, z = .22, p > .05) and between lntemet self-efficacy and support for censorship (error = .02, z = .26, p > .05) indicated that the data were consistent 35 with the model suggested (see Figure 2). The overall fit test also supported that the data were consistent with the model as a whole (x2 (2) = .11, p > .05). These results suggest that individuals’ collectivism and lntemet self-efficacy influence third-person perception, which then leads to support for censorship of lntemet pornography. Figure 2: Path Coefficients among IndividualismlCollectivism, Internet Self- Efficacy, Third-person Perception, and Support for Censorship Collectivism -.15* .27!"A . * Support for Censorship Third-person __, Perception lntemet pornography .13* lntemet Self-Efficacy *p<.05,**p< .01 Overall fit,12(2)= .11,p> .05 Missing path link between collectivism and support for censorship: Error = .02, z = .22, p = .829 Missing path link between lntemet self-efficacy and support for censorship: Error = .02, z = .26, p = .794 36 Chapter 4 Discussion This study examined the influence of cultural individualism/collectivism and lntemet self-efficacy on third-person perception in the context of lntemet pornography, and the relationship between this third-person perception and support for censorship as its behavioral consequence. Consistent with past findings on the third-person effect, participants in this study perceive lntemet pornography to have a greater negative effect on others than on themselves. The findings also supported the behavioral component hypothesis showing that third-person perception was significantly associated with support for censorship. As hypothesized, the influence of individualism/collectivism and lntemet self-efficacy on the third-person perception was also observed. Finally, the results of path analysis suggest that the data were consistent with a causal model in which both individuals’ collectivism and lntemet self-efficacy influence third-person perception, which then leads to support for censorship of lntemet pornography. Tests of hypothesis 1 supported the previous findings on the third-person perceptual hypothesis showing that individuals are likely to perceive lntemet pornography to have a greater negative influence on others than on themselves. Irnportantly, the results replicate previous findings from traditional media and extend them to include lntemet exposure. Tests of hypothesis 2 examined cultural individualism/collectivism as a causal factor of the third-person perception. The negative association found between these two variables demonstrates that as collectivism increases, the perceptual discrepancy between 37 the negative effects of lntemet pornography on others and on self will decrease. For the first time, the present study empirically supported the assumptions of others (Gunther & Hwa, 1996; Lee & Yang, 1996), claiming that people who are high on collectivism are less likely to show third-person perception than people who are low on individualism. Additionally, these data are consistent with arguments that the difference between two cultures associated with self-concept impacts optimistic bias and subsequent perceptual discrepancy. Tests of hypothesis 3 examined the effect of lntemet self-efficacy on the third- person perception. Even though most prior research findings failed to support the assumption that media use is related to the third-person perception, this study found a significant positive association between lntemet self-efficacy and third-person perception. This finding might stem from the fact that unlike traditional media, special skills and knowledge required to use the lntemet magnify its impact on third-person perception. This set of circumstances may enhance the ability of Internet self—efficacy to cause an optimistic bias and lead to this third-person perception. The final hypothesis was to test the behavioral component of the third-person effect. A significant association between third-person perception and support for censorship of lntemet pornography was found, even after controlling for other independent variables. This result strongly supports previous findings showing that as the perceived dependency of media’s negative impact on self and others increases, so too does support for censorship of the media content. This study has several implications. First, it extends previous findings on the third- person effect to the context of lntemet pornography. There is now little doubt that 38 the lntemet firnctions as a crucial medium in our society. As such, there are likely to be several important areas in which the Intemet’s role in the third-person effect phenomena should be studied. This study’s demonstration that media use impacts those third-person effects associated with lntemet content suggests that the factors associated with this phenomenon may be qualitatively different for Internet exposure. In light of the fact that third-person effects can influence public opinion and resulting policy-making processes, studying the third person effects of the lntemet seems critical to understanding those opinions likely to shape future regulation of the most influential medium of our time. This study examined also the influence of people’s collectivism on the third- person effect. Although cultural individualism/collectivism has been considered as an important factor in prior research, this is the first study to demonstrate empirically that individualism/collectivism and third-person perception are related. Though third-person effect studies have focused primarily on Western culture, this study suggests that cultural differences are an important consideration in explaining this phenomenon. Finally, there are several limitations in this study. First, as suggested in most third- person effect research, this study used measures of attitude, not actual response, as the behavioral component of the third-person effect. Clearly, this is a concern. Nevertheless, as stated earlier, since public policy and judicial decisions are based on public opinion, the study of these public attitudes or opinions toward the regulation of socially undesirable media content tends to emerge eventually as a social consequence (Gunther, 1995; McLeod, Eveland, & Nathanson, 1997). Second, the generalization of results is limited since this study used a convenience sample of college students from two countries. In addition to other potential bias, this purposive sampling suppressed the possible role of some 39 lama liter: using cons no \‘31’ IIEI p01 demographic variables (e. g. age, income, education, etc.) in the third—person effect. Therefore, firture study should attempt to replicate the findings suggested in this study using a more representative sample of the general population. Finally, this study did not consider other variables that might influence and explain the third-person effect. The exogenous variables included in this study did not explain a substantial amount of the total variance of third-person effect. For example, other attitudinal variables, such as support for free speech, political affiliation, or religiosity, could be examined in relationship to lntemet pornography and the third-person effect. 40 APPENDIX Questionnaire (English Version) I. In this first set of questions, you will read statement about different aspects of daily life and social relationships. Read each statement carefully and please circle your level of agreement / disagreement how much you agree with that statement. 1. If a husband is a sports fan, a wife should also cultivate an interest in sports. If the husband is a stock broker, the wife should also be aware of the current market situation. Strongly Agree Agree Neutral Disagree Strongly Disagree 1 2 3 4 5 2. If one is interested in a job about which the spouse is not very enthusiastic, one should apply for it anyway. Strongly Agree Agree Neutral Disagree Strongly Disagree 1 2 3 4 5 3. It is better for a husband and wife to have their own bank accounts rather than to have a joint account. Strongly Agree Agree Neutral Disagree Strongly Disagree 1 2 3 4 5 4. The decision of where one is to work should be jointly made with one's spouse, if one is married. Strongly Agree Agree Neutral Disagree Strongly Disagree 1 2 3 4 5 5. In these days parents are too stringent with their kids, stunting the development of initiative. Strongly Agree Agree Neutral Disagree Strongly Disagree 1 2 3 4 5 6. When making important decisions, I seldom consider the positive and negative effects my decisions have on my parents. Strongly Agree Agree Neutral Disagree Strongly Disagree 1 2 3 4 5 41 7. Teenagers should listen to their parents' advice on dating. Strongly Agree Agree Neutral Disagree Strongly Disagree 1 2 3 4 5 8. Even if the child won the Nobel prize, the parents should not feel honored in any way. Strongly Agree Agree Neutral Disagree Strongly Disagree 1 2 3 4 5 9. It is reasonable for a son to continue his father's business. Strongly Agree Agree Neutral Disagree Strongly Disagree 1 2 3 4 5 10. I would not share my ideas and newly acquired knowledge with my parents. Strongly Agree Agree Neutral Disagree Strongly Disagree 1 2 3 4 5 11. I practice the religion of my parents. Strongly Agree Agree Neutral Disagree Strongly Disagree 1 2 3 4 5 12. I would not let my parents use my car (if I have one), whether they are good drivers or not. Strongly Agree Agree Neutral Disagree Strongly Disagree 1 2 3 4 5 13. Children should not feel honored even if the father were highly praised and given an award by a government official for his contribution and service to the community. Strongly Agree Agree Neutral Disagree Strongly Disagree 1 2 3 4 5 14. Young people should take into consideration their parents' advice when making education or career plan. Strongly Agree Agree Neutral Disagree Strongly Disagree 1 2 3 4 5 15. The bigger a family, the more family problems there are. Strongly Agree Agree Neutral Disagree Strongly Disagree 1 2 3 4 5 42 16. I have never told my parents the number of sons I want to have. Strongly Agree Agree Neutral Disagree Strongly Disagree 1 2 3 4 5 17. I would help, within my means, if a relative told me that he/she is in financial difficulty. Strongly Agree Agree Neutral Disagree Strongly Disagree 1 2 3 4 5 18. If I met a person whose last name was the same as mine, I would start wondering whether we were, at lest remotely, related by blood. Strongly Agree Agree Neutral Disagree Strongly Disagree 1 2 3 4 5 19. Whether one spends an income extravagantly or stingily is of no concern to one's relatives (cousins, uncles). Strongly Agree Agree Neutral Disagree Strongly Disagree 1 2 3 4 5 20. I would not let my cousin use my car (If I have one). Strongly Agree Agree Neutral Disagree Strongly Disagree 1 2 3 4 5 21. When deciding what kind of work to do, I would definitely pay attention to the views of relatives of my generation. Strongly Agree Agree Neutral Disagree Strongly Disagree 1 2 3 4 5 22. When deciding what kind of education to have, I would pay absolutely no attention to my uncles' or cousins' advice. Strongly Agree Agree Neutral Disagree Strongly Disagree 1 2 3 4 5 23. Each family has its own problems unique to itself. It does not help to tell relatives about one's problems. Strongly Agree Agree Neutral Disagree Strongly Disagree 1 2 3 4 5 43 24. I can count on my relatives for help if I find myself in any kind of trouble. Strongly Agree Agree Neutral Disagree Strongly Disagree 1 2 3 4 5 25. I have never chatted with my neighbors about the political future of this state Strongly Agree Agree Neutral Disagree Strongly Disagree 1 2 3 4 5 26. I am often influenced by the moods of my neighbors. Strongly Agree Agree Neutral Disagree Strongly Disagree 1 2 3 4 5 27. My neighbors always tell me interesting stories that have happened around them. Strongly Agree Agree Neutral Disagree Strongly Disagree 1 2 3 4 5 28. I am not interested in knowing what my neighbors are really like. Strongly Agree Agree Neutral Disagree Strongly Disagree 1 2 3 4 5 29. One need not worry about what the neighbors say about whom one should marry. Strongly Agree Agree Neutral Disagree Strongly Disagree 1 2 3 4 5 30. I enjoy meeting and talking to my neighbors everyday. Strongly Agree Agree Neutral Disagree Strongly Disagree 1 2 3 4 5 31. In the past, my neighbors have never borrowed anything from me or my family. Strongly Agree Agree Neutral Disagree Strongly Disagree 1 2 3 4 5 32. One needs to be cautious in talking with neighbors, otherwise others might think you are noisy. Strongly Agree Agree Neutral Disagree Strongly Disagree 1 2 3 4 5 44 33. I don't really know how to befiiend my neighbors. Strongly Agree Agree Neutral Disagree Strongly Disagree 1 2 3 4 5 34. I feel uneasy when my neighbors do not greet me when we come across each other. Strongly Agree Agree Neutral Disagree Strongly Disagree 1 2 3 4 5 35. I would rather struggle through a personal problem by myself than discuss it with my fiiends. Strongly Agree Agree Neutral Disagree Strongly Disagree 1 2 3 4 5 36. I like to live close to my good fiiends. Strongly Agree Agree Neutral Disagree Strongly Disagree 1 2 3 4 5 37. My good friends and I agree on the best places to shop. Strongly Agree Agree Neutral Disagree Strongly Disagree 1 2 3 4 5 38. I would pay absolutely no attention to my close friends' views when deciding what kind of work to do. Strongly Agree Agree Neutral Disagree Strongly Disagree 1 2 3 4 5 39. It is a personal matter whether I worship money or not. Therefore, it is not necessary for my fiiends to give any counsel. Strongly Agree Agree Neutral Disagree Strongly Disagree 1 2 3 4 5 40. I have never loaned my car/lap top to any classmates. Strongly Agree Agree Neutral Disagree Strongly Disagree 1 2 3 4 5 45 41. A group of people at their workplace was discussing where to eat. A popular choice was a restaurant which had recently opened. However, someone in the group had discovered that the food there was unpalatable. Yet the group disregarded this person's objection, and insisted on trying it out. There were only two alternatives for the person who objected: either to go or not to go with others. In this situation, not going with the others is a better choice. Strongly Agree Agree Neutral Disagree Strongly Disagree 1 2 3 4 5 42. There is everything to gain and nothing to lose for classmates to group themselves for study and discussion. Strongly Agree Agree Neutral Disagree Strongly Disagree 1 2 3 4 5 43. Classmates' assistance is indispensable to getting a good grade at school. Strongly Agree Agree Neutral Disagree Strongly Disagree 1 2 3 4 5 44. I would help if a classmate at school told me that he/she needed money to pay something. Strongly Agree Agree Neutral Disagree Strongly Disagree 1 2 3 4 5 45. In most cases, to cooperate with someone whose ability is lower than one's own is not as desirable as doing the thing alone. Strongly Agree Agree Neutral Disagree Strongly Disagree 1 2 3 4 5 II. The next questions ask you some questions about your perception or behavior regarding Internet use. 46. On average, how many hours a week do you use lntemet regardless of personal use or professional use? hours 47. I feel confident understanding terms/words relating to Internet hardware. Strongly Agree Agree Neutral Disagree Strongly Disagree 1 2 3 4 5 46 48. I feel confident understanding terms/words relating to lntemet software. Strongly Agree Agree Neutral Disagree Strongly Disagree 1 2 3 4 5 49. I feel confident describing functions of lntemet hardware. Strongly Agree Agree Neutral Disagree Strongly Disagree 1 2 3 4 5 50. I feel confident trouble shooting lntemet problems. Strongly Agree Agree Neutral Disagree Strongly Disagree 1 2 3 4 5 51. I feel confident explaining why a task will not run on the lntemet. Strongly Agree Agree Neutral Disagree Strongly Disagree 1 2 3 4 5 52. I feel confident using the lntemet to gather data. Strongly Agree Agree Neutral Disagree Strongly Disagree 1 2 3 4 5 53. I feel confident learning advanced skills within a specific Internet program. Strongly Agree Agree Neutral Disagree Strongly Disagree 1 2 3 4 5 54. I feel confident turning to on-line discussion group when help is needed. Strongly Agree Agree Neutral Disagree Strongly Disagree 1 2 3 4 5 III. The next two questions ask you some questions about your experience on Internet pornography. 55. Have you seen or come across pornographic content on the lntemet? Yes No ----- > Skip Q57. 47 56. If yes, how many times have you seen or come across pornographic content on the lntemet since January, 2001? times IV. The next set of questions asks about your general opinions or perception about Internet pornography. Please answer the following questions even if you have never seen or come across Internet pornography. Please indicate your level of agreement/ disagreement. 57. The pornographic content on the lntemet has a negative effect on my moral values concerning sex. Strongly Agree Neutral Strongly Disagree 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 58. The pornographic content on the lntemet has a negative effect on my attitudes toward the opposite sex. Strongly Agree Neutral Strongly Disagree 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 59. The pornographic content on the lntemet has a negative effect on other people's moral values concerning sex. Strongly Agree Neutral Strongly Disagree 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 60. The pornographic content on the lntemet has a negative effect on other people's attitudes toward the opposite sex. Strongly Agree Neutral Strongly Disagree 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 61. Pornography sites on the lntemet should be censored. Strongly Agree Neutral Strongly Disagree 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 62. Today's standards for regulating pornography site on the lntemet are too strict. Strongly Agree Neutral Strongly Disagree 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 48 63. The freedom of expression of people who post pornographic materials on the lntemet should be restricted. Strongly Agree Neutral Strongly Disagree 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 64. promography site on the lntemet has an appropriate device to block the access of children, it should not be restricted. Strongly Agree Neutral Strongly Disagree 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 V. Finally, We would like to know a little bit about you. Please answer the following questions. 65. What is your gender? (Check one) Male Female 66. What is your age? years 67. With which of the following groups or group do you most identify? Cl African-American Cl Asian-American or Pacific Islander Cl Latino Cl Native American Cl White Cl Prefer not to answer Cl Other Thank you for your valuable participation in this survey. 49 Questionnaire (Korean Version) QlEl‘fl 01-8-34 913% £§i°ll 3&1} ”SE-25.4} r. creel IIWEIE- eeei ale—se- Aiee—aa aw area 14317-1 anion :Ie 319.11%}. are an age at 2 751201 man were OI}:— 78E are are emaiaezls Marci #2471 via bit}. 1. ‘it? 01% EEO] ii}: 1801 EVE :1 ONIE SE 12$qu Ell-(slot 123% 7IX‘IOI (5113}. 3.11.11 {‘14. E3591 31' 0]%fl Eiflai‘fl :1 0N1 93*] P1111194 *l’éi %-§i°ll PISS: 7l%°fil°l= {Elli}. DH?- %9|P=* %9—|’.§I §El t‘JEII El 0H5?- ‘EJEH Pet 1 2 3 4 5 2. Olifi *ltiol ilfilE 318.10] 915131., 43./“I :1 HIWZPI H81 ‘BziEIEIE :1 3121-8: Q7] 94311-841311 of EDI. one egg: sale ea ”Jill er one ELHlH a 1 2 3 4 5 3 exam owl—:- %%9I SSIHEIE Mamie 7424 new eerae are 7.101%EI one esale gala ea ear :2: one trials 1 2 3 4 5 4. viewers. xlaa 347801] area aee ans—w are new eat one sale sew ea aura one Eml a l 2 3 4 5 5 gee ease xILI-s—el ewe new 71 7lEEIE— LII-r 217—16I7l‘i'. 6ch uls— ERIE sale ea ear 22 oil-r- FLIEH a 1 2 3 4 5 6. eat} aes— LIe rrI, LR:— :1 E7310] esteem uni-axle as— we ase- ezgz—I eaten a sIoI a: maxi ere—cl. UH Eglai E 0 C3 0 ....-IO 50 seeO' 4 Par—non fi%7l%01<>t EDI 91 til OI £5.01] 3&3“ *1 :15— E 20 at: 7.10:1 In! .6D mu 5 2. 1.! 8a W4 UL €21 an o. 2 so an cl. :3 2. 1 E8151. $3513} 311313 lineanzamaea 0 33319-1? 5.1-2.8. .Q. a at!A IOIREI 0l017llrsé‘el Ir 8a Ir ES 2. Ir .am W 4 5.. LI 8a 0.2 So If .65 5o 2. l or... 9. Kit-101 OIBIXI 91 ”+21 Ir an m: 5 2. In! an W4 U.— Ir Sn 0. 2 go as 0. so 2. 1 LII7IAH§ t «AH *7 Ila SWOILIZIéIS-LII—PE‘EUI —1 %L%5}X] 0142.7} L'O'E'. A015}. 10. LIE- Ir to ma 5 2. LI 8c W4 5.. Ir 6a 0. 2 go mm o. 50 gr I '73. " JI—a ELITE 72301:} Ma an a. E In! to ma 5 2. In! fig W4 U.— LI .GD 0. 2 5o l—r 8a 50 2. 1 EEI ZI saaaasuwweeEHAJ D ea 1:. O 12.03%! ' 04 T 4— LH‘dIHIZVI 214..)"E ‘ l lac $140210] \_.L.] fix ofia 1r 0 Is! 8s m... 5 2. Ir 8a W4 H; Egg 0..- Ir 8a :3 2. I fix :1. or Edam :L 2i“ _, oi El Sit} E. E, 110 13. BIS 0 M2171 21914331011 tll '1 as. SE9} SHIP. Oil Ell‘<"SIl 0333521 $1 61W CHE}. ._‘-:_ L. LI 6a .Hu 5 2. LI 6n W4 H.— ID! as o. 2 _._._o as oz 50 2. 1 Ir Sn .3 5 0T Ll 6a W4 H._ Ir 85 o. 2 5o ma 5o 0T 1 If 6n .3 5 0T as W4 Ht Ewe C)" an .9. go 2. 1 51 are -a TluaTeaamaaeamewI reeuvwraes 16. LIE 0 I»! so ma 5 2. Ir 6a W4 U.— I»! an 0. 2 2o .2. 0. so 2. 1 17.91%} LII ilio Alt-121131 0181822 Lila] F201 22.-Elsi: LIME Eeaeq Ir 2°C 022 so as 20 2. 1 BIB] Emil ._ JIM 20M 7l77l—3— 2]? £1 a} wenee “11.1%:- 3 L 1015}. .‘1‘. L. 212 LI an .3 5 2. If an W4 B.— If ._0D 0.2 So an 0. 20 2. 1 l9 i131} :LO 3 3: .1 m. a t .1 . u , a 4525).)“ 75123143 014136171] 5).: a 0 $512 L- L. I -._..] :LA |___._-o] OI] ‘A aaauneaeanquq Ir .32 me 5 2. lb! .02 W4 H1 §EI 0117“ '23-?“ 120L274 Lea A01EI'. mg 022 2° Ir 3m 0. so 2. 1 32; L. *I E ._. E 2 DO o 57% EJJEE 7mg. 56. 2124 Jag-WE, $1545 %2001€1%¥E1 9? 2% 2310114 3.753% y.*lflb} fifiW-fi-HW}? 3'11 56 IV. 11173-43.- °1EI‘51’2}91 £91: 1%: 1%?315-41 ‘11?! 4181-4 §3J31°J91213194é11%= QE- {vi-ii} 54211451 “1E 413171°J51fl ’89] iii-'9‘- 4% %fl%% 75?} 78§°1 8111131131315- %‘&31} EH] 7] 31%}‘4‘4 1114511". 2J€E°1I £11121 %«1 iii-€- ‘i‘lEH-g] 785% fifilfidl -’r‘-’~l 7] ”1'3‘451. 57. 9151 L11 ”8191 Sigh-.110: %%1% 08130195 “1’2 7530”}. r10 L194 *JEEEL, 5% 1801] 336% 5W1/zral 1* 7121011 233:! 5. 2 [101' 01)! [IE FE EH33 OH?- 0H?- %9| 1 7 b) .5}. Ln ON 58°JE151’8191EME— §%%?}%~8—LH7177<131°1—‘501*é°11583113511qu1:11-2831‘1813619513123101 51. DH ._..qo %9|3.=1 §El ”HS?- ‘E‘JCH E1 2 4 b.) 59. 01H L1 ”91 £8 1:—_, 9: %%}%€: 94%flii 51% ”Fa/1&9] 183%}, % 18011112318 $53.34/$1— a123, 7}x101]—?—131—1 086018: ”1%; 33015}. 0H5? %9I§ €52! UH? “JEN?! 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 60 (JEN ”81-91 £51: 3% 3.1%:- (QEMEE the: A1%%°1 7x131 alv‘: 01801] 12:1 51150111? 78711. °3601% 131%! 330151. UHFT’ %9I J §El 0H5? tJJLHEI 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 61. 9113191 891 £221: 401.12% 615.81 73%5101018} 6m. UH %9l E1 §a=l OHS?- E‘JEH E1 2 3 4 7 M O‘ 62 3: 31.13111 *0191 EL?- E: *MEE fiflfla— 71—i—g. Lil-,1- 119. 2% am. E1H $943+ 0 D a DH? 7 [E 1EH 21 5....‘10 A01)! 2 9.) Ln 0‘ 63.911511}! ”81°11iéi s—g— gal-:2 111%1-3‘91 Efigl 1}%E- 2111121510101: 12.1 w. 0H?- %9I§ éfl 0H5?- BJEH E1 m 2 3 4 5 6 7 57 64.13%1 2112M 6121 £21: 40127} wax—494 set-a- 24 @6171] 911% 4: 551—:- 621% 851 9m :5 :1 4015-1:— fizfl ENE <21 8:}. 1" DH 59's 0 ...-10 §E 0H5?- E‘JF—H 3.21 2 3 4 v. 332$ 418194 41801! {1%} Q 712] QE—dl %"a1311-’1‘-*1‘d #6181386145}. 65. 4'1 6191 182%}? ‘38 04 ’8 663113191 (251.98%? ‘3} *1] @1554! €451] 215114 11111331 34211—1131. 58 REFERENCES Agresti, A. & Finlay, B. (1997). Statistical methods for the social sciences (3rd ed. 1. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice-Hall Inc. Bancroft, L. & Mathews, A. (1971). Autonomic correlates of penile erection. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 15, 159 — 167. Beaver, W. (2000). The dilemma of lntemet pornography. Business and Society Review. 105(3). 373 - 382. Boynton, L. A. & Wu, H. D. (1999, August). Don't loolgat me! Third-person effects and TV violence. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Association for Education in Journalism and Mass Communication, New Orleans, LA. Brosius, H. -B. & Engel, D. (1996). The causes of third-person effects: Unrealistic optimism, impersonal impact, or generalized negative attitudes toward media influence? International Journal of Public opinion Research, 8(2), 142 ~162. Bryant, J. & Zillmann, D. (1996). Violence and sex in the media. In M. B. Salwen, and D. W. Stacks (Eds.), An integrated Approach to communication theory and research (pp. 195 — 209), Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Association, Inc., Publishers. Cate, F. H. (1996). Cybersex: regulating sexually explicit expression on the Internet. Behavioral Sciences and the Law, 14(2), 145 - 166. Chapin, J. R. (2000). Third-person perception and optimistic bias among urban minority at-risk youth. Communication Research, 27(1). 51 - 81. Digital Chosun. (2000). Top 10 News Stories of the Information and Technology Indusfl. December 22. Cohen, J ., Mutz, D., Price, V., & Gunther, A. (1988). Perceived impact of defamation: An experiment on third-person effects. Public Opinion Quarterly, 52, 161 - 173. Davison, W. P. (1983). The third-person effect in communication. Public Opinion QuarterLY.47(1L 1 - 15. Dearing, J. W. & Rogers, E. M. (1996). Agenda-setting. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Donnerstein, E. & Berkowitz, L. (1981). Victim reactions in aggressive erotic films as a factor in violence against women. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 41, 710 — 724. 59 Duck, J. M., Hogg, M. A., & Terry, D. J. (1999). Social identity and perceptions of media persuasion: Are we always less influenced than others? J oumalgof Applied Social Psychology. 29(9). 1879 - 1899. Duck, J. M. and Mullin, B. A. (1995). The perceived impact of the mass media: Reconsidering the third-person effect. European J om] of Social Psychology, 25(1), 77 - 93. Duck, J. M., Terry, D. J ., & Hogg, M. A. (1995). The perceived influence of AIDS advertising: Third-person effects in the context of positive media content. Basic ad Applied SociQPsychology, 17(3), 305 - 325. Dupagne, M., Salwen, M., & Paul, B. (1999). Impact of question order on the third- person effect. International Journal of Public Opinion ReseaLch, 11(4). 334 - 345. Eichholz, M. (1999, August). The third-person effect and the Hiegrchy of communication effects: The perceivedgersuasive power of public relations. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Association for Education in Journalism and Mass Communication, New Orleans, LA. Eastin, M. S. and LaRose, R. (2000). lntemet self-efficacy and the psychology of the digital divide. J ouml of Computer-Mediated Communication [On-line], 6(1). Available: http://www.ascusc.org/jcmc/vol6/issue1/eastin.html. Greenberg, B. S. and Salwen, M. B. (1996). Mass communication theory and research: Concepts and models. In M. B. Salwen, and D. W. Stacks (Eds.), An__ integrated Approach to communication theory and research (pp. 63 — 78), Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Association, Inc., Publishers. Gudykunst, W., Matsumoto, Y., Ting-toomey, S., Nishida, T., Kim, K. S., & Heyman, S. (1996). The influence of cultural individulism-collectivism, self-construals, and individual values on communication styles across cultures. Human Communication Research, 22(4), 510 - 543. Gunther, A. C. (1991). What we think others think: Cause and consequence in the third-person effect. Communication ResearchJI 8(3), 355 - 372. Gunther, A. C. (1995). Overrating the X-rating: The third-person perception and support for censorship of pornography. Journal of Communication 45(1), 27 - 38. Gunther, A. C. & Hwa, A. P. (1996). Public perceptions of television influence and opinions about censorship in Singapore. Interaational Journal of Public Opinion Research, 813 ), 248 — 265. Gunther and Mundy (1993). Biased optimism and the third-person effect. Jomlism Quarterly. 70(1), 58 - 67. 6O Gunther, A. C. & Thorson, E. (1992). Perceived persuasive effects of product commercials and public service announcements: The third-person effects in new domains. Communication Research. 19(5), 574 - 596. Gutfeld, G. (1999). The sex drive. Men's Health, 14(8), 116 -120. Henriksen, L. & Flora, J. A. (1999). Third-person perception and children: Perceived impact of pro- and anti-smoking ads. Communication Research, 26(6), 643 - 665. Hoffner, C., Buchanan, M., Anderson, J. D., Hubbs, L. A., Kamigaki, S. K., Kowalczyk, L., Pastorek, A., Plotkin, R. S., & Silberg, K. J. (1999). Support for censorship of television violence: The role of the third-person effect and news exposure. Communication Research, 26(6), 726 — 742. Hofstede, G. (1991). Cultures and gganizations: Soflware of the mind. London: McGraw-Hill. Hui, C. H. (1988). Measurement of individualism-collectivism. Journal of Research in Personality, 22, 17-36. Hunter, J. E. & Gerbing, D. W. (1982). Machiavellian beliefs and personality: Construct invalidity of the Machiavellianism dimension. J om] of Personality and Social Psychology. 43. 1293 -— 1305. Lambe, J. L. & Shah, D. V. (1999, August). Me speech and the third-person effect: Suscgptibility, severig, and the willingaess to censor. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Association for Education in Journalism and Mass Communication, New Orleans, LA. Lee, C. & Yang, S. (1996, August). Third-person perception and support for censorship of sexually explicit visual content: A Korean case. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Association for Education in Journalism and Mass Communication, Anaheim, CA. Markus, H. R. and Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 98(2), 224 - 253. Marshall, T. R. (1988). Public opinion, representation, and the modern Supreme Court. American Politics Quarterly, 16, 293 — 316. McLeod, D. M., Eveland, W. P. & Nathanson, A. I. (1997). Support for censorship of violent and misogynic rap lyrics: An analysis of the third-person effect. Communication Research, 24(2), 153 — 174. 61 Mehta, M. D. & Plaza, D. (1997). Content analysis of pornographic images available on the lntemet. The Infonfltion Society. 13, 153 -161. Melillo, W. (1999). Policing the net. Adweek, January 4. Mishler, W. & Sheehan, R. S. (1993). The Supreme Court as a countermajoritarian institution? The impact of public opinion on Supreme Court decisions. American Political Science Review, 87, 87 — 101. Munro, N. (1999, September). The Web’s pomucopia. National Journal, 38 — 45. Page, B. I. & Shapiro, R. Y. (1983). Effects of public opinion on policy. American Political Science Review, 77, 175 — 190. Park, E. (1998). IndivicMism/collectivism, self-concepL_ and social behavior: F alse-uniqueness and the spiral of silence hypotheses. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Cornell University. Paxton, M. (1996, August). The third-person effect and attitudes toward expression. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Association for Education in Journalism and Mass Communication, Anaheim, CA. Paxton, M. (1997, March). Mega use, "experts" and the third-person effect. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Association for Education in Journalism and Mass Communication, Southeast Colloquium, Knoxville, TN. Peiser, W. & Peter, J. (2001). Explaining individual differences in third-person perception: A limits/possibilities perspective. Communication Research. 28(2). 156 — 180. Price, V., Huang, L., & Tewksbury, D. (1997). Third-person effects of news coverage: Orientations toward media. J ournalismfiand Mass Communication Quarterly, 74(3), 525 - 540. Rimm, M. (1995). Marketing pornography on the Information Superhighway: A survey of 917,410 images, descriptions, short stories, and animations downloaded 8.5 million times by consumers in over 2000 cities in forty countries, provinces, and territories. Georgetown Law iournaflj3(5), 1849 - 1934. Reno v. ACLU, 117 S. Ct. 2329 (1997). Rojas, H., Shah, D. V., & Faber, R. J. (1997). For the good of others: Censorship and the third-person effect. International J om] of Public Opinion Research, 8(2), 163 — 186. 62 Rucinski, D. & Salmon, C. T. (1990). The ‘Other’ as the vulnerable voter: A study of the third-person effect in the 1988 US. presidential campaign. International J oumal of Public Opinion Research. 2(4). 345 — 368. Salwen, M. B. (1998). Perceptions of media influence and support for censorship: The third-person effect in the 1996 presidential election. Communication Research, 25(3), 259 — 285. Salwen, M. B. & Driscoll, P. D. (1997). Consequences of third-person perception in support of press restrictions in the O. J. Simpson trial. Journal of Communication, 432), 60 —78. Salwen, M. B. & Dupagne, M. (1999). The third-person effect. Communication Research, 26(5), 523 — 549. Sanders, G., Deal, J ., & Myers-Bowman, K. (1999). Sexually explicit material on the lntemet: Implications for family life educators. Journal of Family aad Consumer Sciences, 91(3), 112 - 115. Simon, G. E. (1998). Cyberpom and censorship: Constitutional barriers to preventing access to lntemet pornography by minors. The Journal of Criminal Law & Criminology, 88(3), 1015 — 1048. Singelis, T. M., & Brown, W. J. (1995). Culture, self, and collectivist communication: Linking culture to individual behavior. Human Communication Research, 21, 354 - 389. Slade, J. W. (2000). Pornograpm' in America: A reference handbook. Santa Barbara, CA: ABC-CLIO, Inc. Stanford Institute for the Quantitative Study of Society (SIQSS). (2000, February). Internet and socieg: A preliminm rgport. Stanford University. The Washington Post. (2000). Koreans clash on lntemet sex video: Traditional defend old values in 'wiredfirtion. December 25. Tiedge, J. T., Silverblatt, A., Havice, M. J ., & Rosenfield, R. (1991). Discrepancy between perceived first-person and perceived third-person mass media effects. Journalism Quarterly, 68, 141 - 154. Ting-Toomey, S. (1999). Communicating across cultures. New York, NY: Guilford Press. Vandiver, D. M. (2000). Regulating lntemet pornography. Crime & Justice htemtional. 16(47). 11 - 14. 63 Weimann, G. (2000). Communicatig unreality: Modern media aral the reconstruction of reality. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Weinstein, N. D. (1980). Unrealistic optimism about future life events. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 802 - 820. Youn, S., Faber, R. J ., & Shah, D. V. (2000). Restricting gambling advertising and the third-person effect. Psychologyaml Marketing, 17(7). 633 — 649. Zillrnann, D. & Bryant, J. (1982). Pornography, sexual callousness, and the trivialization of rape. Journal of Communication, 32, 10 — 21. Zillrnann, D., Bryant, J ., Comisky, P. W., & Medoff, N. J. (1981). Excitation and hedonic valence in the effect of erotica on motivated intennale aggression. European Journal of Social Psychology. 11, 233 — 252. Zillrnann, D., Bryant, J. & Huston, A. C. (1994). Mediaachildrengand the family: Social scientific. psychodynamic, and clinicai perspectives. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Association, Inc., Publishers. Zillrnann, D. & Weaver, J. B. (1989). Pornography and men’s sexual callouseness toward women. In D. Zillrnann and J. Bryant (Eds), Pomography: Research advances and policy considerations (pp. 95 — 125), Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Association, Inc., Publishers. G 1111111111111111111151111