; :1- u‘ xol‘v . k Ad.“- maa ”a. fine. .. 2%., .2sz :ths “16h “ AG. In :x'. I it \v;5. .. 1.1;. .PYiI}: In l t .6.- 1.» hsafiufi....: 91..» I1 u; )1: 2 u .1!) u 0. {A I! tag-\x» '1‘,- .l.j)fl”',YtOI.I-.3-. on.“ r .3 . : . . . . .. . . . . 3...)... Ltd. EERE . $373.32 H : .. . _ .. . . . . n.3,..-é: a. I (,1 lIBRARY Michigan State University This is to certify that the dissertation entitled FACTORS RELATED TO INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES IN THE ACADEMIC AND BEHAVIORAL ADJUSTMENT OF YOUNG CHILDREN FROM LOW-INCOME FAMILIES presented by Bye-Won Hwang has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for Ph.D. . Family and Child Ecology degree in may ,4. PM; Major professor 21 01 Date 9/ / MS U i: an Affirmative Action/Equal Opportunity Institution 0-12771 PLACE IN RETURN BOX to remove this checkout from your record. TO AVOID FINES return on or before date due. MAY BE RECALLED with earlier due date if requested. DATE DUE DATE DUE DATE DUE Mama) 5 29163 mtg! 6/01 cJClRC/DaieDuepGS—p. 15 FACTORS RELATED TO INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES IN THE ACADEMIC AND BEHAVIORAL ADJUSTMENT OF YOUNG CHILDREN FROM LOW-INCOME FAMILIES By Hye-Won Hwang A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Family and Child Ecology 2001 ABSTRACT FACTORS RELATED TO INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES IN THE ACADEMIC AND BEHAVIORAL ADJUSTMENT OF YOUNG CHILDREN FROM LOW-INCOME FAMILIES By Hye—Won Hwang The purpose Of this study was to identify factors that predict individual differences in the academic and behavioral adjustment Of children from low- income families. This study also examined the parenting process of low-income families, predictors of maternal behavior, and the effects of maternal parenting behavior on the developmental outcomes of children from low-income families. Specifically, this study investigated the relationships among child characteristics, maternal characteristics, maternal psychological well-being, contextual factors, maternal behavior, and Children’s academic achievement and behavioral problems. Using data from the 1994 wave of the National Longitudinal Survey of Youth (NLSY), the study focused on 291 mothers and their 5 to 8~year-old children. Descriptive analysis, zero—order correlations, multiple regression analyses, t-tests, and Chi-square analyses were used for data description and analysis. A structural equation model (SEM) was used to test the conceptual model for this study. Consistent with Bronfenbrenner’s ecological model, the study shows that the children develop in the relationships within various kinds of environments such as the Child’s family, neighborhood, and child care setting. Maternal psychological well-being was affected by the mother’s residence with both parents until her 18th birthday and her perception of neighborhood problems. The results of this study were also consistent with Belsky’s model of the determinants of parenting. The quality of the home environment that mothers provided for their children was influenced by multiple factors, such as child characteristics, maternal characteristics, and contextual factors. Academically successful children from low-income families had grandmothers who were more educated and mothers with higher intelligence scores. Their mothers provided a more supportive home environment. Successful children in terms of behavioral adjustment tended to have heavier birth weights, mothers with higher level of psychological well-being, and better quality home environments. Copyright by HYE WON HWANG 2001 To God To my parents ACKNOWLEDGMENTS I want to express my appreciation to many who made possible the completion of my doctoral program as well as this dissertation. First, I thank God. WIthout his grace and mercy, this work would not have been possible. I would like to extend my Sincere appreciation to Dr. Phenice, chairperson Of my doctoral guidance committee, for her assistance with the development of this work. From the very beginning of my work at Michigan State University, she encouraged me to continue to study abroad and took care of me like a parent. I will never forget her kindness and understanding for me. I also thank other members of my guidance committee, Dr. Griffore, Dr. Luster, and Dr. Edwards, for their contributions in both my study and research. Their support and trust in my ability to learn gave me the determination to complete my doctoral program and this dissertation. I offer special thanks and love to my mother, Young-0k Kim, and my father, Khen-Shik Hwang. Their endless love, encouragement, and prayers have helped me to complete my program in the United States. I also thank my parents-in-law for their prayers and understanding for me. Deep appreciation is extended to my husband, Sukjoon Hong, for waiting patiently for me to finish my work. He willingly endured the separation with me for two years and encouraged me to be a good scholar. I also thank my baby girl, Jimin Hong, for doing well inside Of me while I wrote this dissertation. Finally, I thank my friends who provided loving support and prayers. vi TABLE OF CONTENTS LISTOFTABLES ix LISTOFFIGURES xi CAHPTER1 INTRODUCTION... . StatementofthePrObIem MainResearchQuestions...w.................................................... Research Objectives... TheoreticalFramework . Bronfenbrenner’ S ECOIOgicaI Perspective Of Human Development... . Belsky’ 5 Model of the Determinants of Parenting BehaVIor McLoyd’ S Analytic Model of How Poverty Affects Children ..... 12 Garmezy's Work on the Protective Factors of Poor Children. 15 ABridgingOfConcepts.................................................. 15 ConceptualMap... . 21 ConceptualandOperationalDefinitions 23 Assumptions... 25 (.IIU'I-b-h-ib (00) CHAPTER 2 REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE... .. 26 Factors Related to Maternal Psychological W'eII- -Being and Behavior... 26 Factors Contributed to lndividUaI Differences among Young Children from Low-Income Families... 32 CHAPTER3 METHODOLOGY... 50 ResearcthpOtheses... 50 ResearchDesignandPrOCedure 58 SampleSelection... 59 SampleDescriptionw...”.......................................................... 60 Research Instruments 63 DataAnaIyseS 69 CHAPTER 4 RESULTSAND DISCUSSION. 71 Relations among the PrediCtOr VariabIeS .. 71 Relation between Predictor Variables and the Psychological Well- -being of the Mother... .. .. 74 Relations between Predictor Variable and the Ouality Of the Home Environment... 75 Multiple Predictors Of the Maternal Psychological Well- -being .......... 80 vii Multiple Predictors of the Quality of the Home Environment ............ Relations between the Predictor Variables and the Academic and Behavioral Adjustment of Children...... Multiple Regression Analyses... . Differences in the Circumstances Of the Children Who SCOredm in the Top Quartile and Bottom Quartiles on the PIAT and BPI.. Other Differences between the Successful Group and the Comparison Group... . Structural Equation Modeling of the ConCeptuaI MOdeI for this Study... Summary Of Results... . . . . .. ... . CHAPTER 5 SUMMARY, CONCLUSIONS, LIMITATIONS, IMPLICATIONS, AND SUGGESTIONS FOR FUTURE RESEARCH... .. . Summary Ofthe Study... Conclusions... Limitations... Implications Suggestions fOr FUture Research APPENDIXA—A Measure Ofthe Marital Quality APPENDIX B — A Measure of Maternal Psychological Well-being... APPENDIX C - Measures of the Home Environment... APPENDIX D - A Measure Of the Neighborhood Environment... .... viii 82 84 88 92 95 99 1 05 ... 115 115 119 124 125 . 128 134 138 141 145 148 LIST OF TABLES Table 1 Sample Characteristics.............................................. Table 2 Relations among the Predictor Variables................................. Table 3 Zero-order Correlations: The Relations between the Predictor Variables and the Psychological Well-being of the Mother ........... Table 4 Zero-order Correlations: The Relations between the Predictor Variables and the Quality of the Home Environment (HOME 1994)..................... Table 5 T-test for Differences between African-American/Hispanic and Non-Black/Non-Hispanic Subsamples in Demographic Characteristics Table 6 Chi-square Analyses for Differences between African-American/ Hispanic and Non-BIaCk/Non-Hispanic Subsamples... Table 7 Multiple Regression Analyses: Predictors Of the Maternal Psychological Well-being (CESD) Table 8 Multiple Regression Analyses: Predictors of the Quality of the Home Environment. Table 9 Relations between the Predictor Variables and the Adjustment OfChildren........................ Table 10 Multiple Regression Analyses: Predictors of the Adjustment of Children. Table 11 T-test for Differences between the Successful Group and the Comparison Group on the PIAT Reading Recognition in Demographic Characteristics... Table 12 Chi-square Analyses for Differences between the Successful Group and the Comparison Group on the PIAT Reading Recognition. .. Table 13 T-test for Differences between the Successful Group and the Comparison Group on the BPI in Demographic Characteristics... Table 14 Chi-square Analyses for Differences between the Successful Group and the Comparison Group on the BPI ix 61 72 75 76 78 79 81 83 85 89 93 93 94 94 Table 15 T-test for Differences between the Successful Group and the Comparison Group on the PIAT Reading Recognition in Other Characteristics ................................................................... 96 Table 16 T-test for Differences between the Successful Group and the Comparison Group on the BPI in Other Characteristics .............. 98 Table 17 Structural Model: Parameter Estimates, Critical Ratios, and Model fits ......................................................................... 104 LIST OF FIGURES Figure 1 Bronfenbrenner’s Model of the Human Ecosystem... 7 Figure 2 A Process Model of the Determinants Of Parenting... 10 Figure 3 Analytic Model of How Poverty and Economic LOSS Affect Black Children 13 Figure4 Theoretical Framework 16 Figure5 Conceptual Map 22 Figure6 TheStructural Model 101 Figure 7 The Revised Structural Model 102 Chapter 1 INTRODUCTION Although many researchers and practitioners as well as governing bodies have tried to find some ways to reduce the poverty rate, there are still 13.5 million children or about 20% Of children in America who are living in poverty (Children’s Defense Fund, 2000). Many previous studies have Shown that children in poverty are more likely to be at risk in all developmental areas. According to McLoyd (1998), poverty experienced during early childhood disturbs the development Of school-readiness Skills and socioemotional functioning. Therefore, this study investigates the factors that influence the development of children in poverty. Poverty thresholds differ by family size and are adjusted annually for changes in the average cost of living in the United States. In 1994, the estimated poverty line was $11,821 for a family Of three and $15,141 for a family Of four (Statistical Abstract of the United States, 1999). It is imperative to note that classifying segments of the population as either poor or nonpoor oversimplifies the economic circumstances of many individuals. Not only the experiences of the long-term poor are quite different from individuals who are poor only for short periods, but also some families have incomes falling below 50% of the poverty line. In addition, there are many near poor who have incomes between 100% and 185% of the poverty line although they are not Officially considered as poor. Experts of this matter indicate that families near poverty may have as much or more difficulty than Officially poor families in obtaining food, Shelter, and medical care, as well as other basic goods and services because fewer government programs are available for the near poor families (National Center for Children in Poverty, 1996). An interesting finding is that there is a considerable amount of evidence that not all children from low-income families have poor academic and psychosocial competencies (Luster et al., 2000). There are also children from low-income families who do well in school and are successful in terms Of social and emotional development. Although Children have in common their families’ lower economic resources, they vary on a number Of other characteristics known to have an important influence on the well-being of children. It may include neighborhood quality, health, parenting practices, and parental psychological well-being. Data from this study could be beneficial in providing further evidence factors that contribute to the academic and psychosocial success of Children from low-income families. The result of this study will attempt to provide evidence to understand what makes them different. If more is known about this, scholars and practitioners will be able to plan more effective programs for families and children in poverty. In general, mothers in poverty can be supported in two important ways. One is in the form of material support and the other is through psychological support. When comparing existing studies Of material and psychological support, relatively more studies have focused on the material support for mothers. More information is needed from studies that focus on the psychological well-being of low-income mothers. Previous studies Show that low-income mothers tend to have higher levels of depression than middle or high-income mothers, and among the highest rates Of depression have been found among low-income mothers with young children. Results Of several studies with this population have reported that from 45 to 60 percent of low-income mothers report clinically Significant levels of depressive symptoms (Hall et al., 1991; McLoyd et al., 1994). These rates are more than double that found in surveys of the general population. However not all low-income mothers are psychologically depressed (Coiro, 1997). This study will investigate factors that may influence low-income mothers’ psychological well-being. It is believed that psychologically healthy mothers are more likely to provide better home environments for their children than depressed mothers. This study can provide evidence to support or nOt support that commonly held belief. Most of the previous studies have demonstrated a simple association between families’ economic stress and the well-being of children. There is little research that focuses on intervening mechanisms between families’ economic situations and children’s outcome, such as maternal behavior and family relationships. This study focuses on identifying the factors that mediate the effects of poverty on development. It is both of scientific and practical value to understand the complex mechanisms through which poverty influences individuals and how the process of parenting is changed by the context Of poverty (Kaiser & Delaney, 1996). Statement Of the Problem The purpose of this study is to identify factors that predict individual differences in the academic and behavioral adjustment Of 5 to 8 year-Old children from low-income families. Specifically, this study will investigate the relationships among child Characteristics, maternal characteristics, maternal psychological well-being, contextual factors, maternal behavior (quality Of home environment that mothers provide for their children), and Children’s reading Skills and behavior problems. Main Research Questions 1. What are the factors that predict individual differences in academic and behavioral adjustment of young Children from low-income families? 2. What are the factors that influence low-income mothers’ psychological well-. being? 3. What effect does maternal psychological well-being have on the academic and behavioral adjustment of young children from low-income families? 4. What are the factors that influence low-income mothers’ parenting behavior (quality Of the home environment mothers provide)? 5. What effect does mothers’ parenting behavior have on the academic and behavioral adjustment of young children from low-income families? Research Objectives 1. TO determine what maternal characteristics and contextual factors are associated with maternal psychological well-being. 2. To determine what Child characteristics, maternal characteristics, and contextual factors are associated with the quality Of the home environment mothers provide. 3. To determine if maternal psychological well-being is related to the quality of the home environment mothers provide. 4. To determine if the quality Of the home environment is related to children’s academic and behavioral adjustment. 5. To determine the relations between the predictor variables (child characteristics, maternal Characteristics, contextual factors, and maternal psychological well-being) and children’s academic and behavioral adjustment, when the quality of the home environment is statistically controlled. 6. To identify what factors contribute to successful outcomes in young Children from low-income families. Theoretical Framework Bronfenbrenner’s ecology of human development is the background theory Of this study. Belsky’s model of the determinants of parenting behavior, McLoyd’s analytic model of how poverty and economic loss affect Children, and Garmezy’s work in investigating the protective factors of poor children are used as primary theoretical frameworks for this study. Bronfenbrenner’s Ecological Perspective of Huma_n. Development The ecology of human development by Bronfenbrenner is used as the background theory of this study. He asserts that human development research Should include an awareness of the environmental systems within which people are operating. Families and children live in a variety of physical and social environments. The ecology of human development involves the scientific study of the progressive, mutual accommodation between an active, growing human being and the changing properties of the immediate settings in which the developing person lives, as this process is affected by relations between these settings, and by the larger contexts in which the settings are embedded (Bronfenbrenner, 1979, p. 21). Bronfenbrenner’s (1979, 1986) model of the human ecology focuses upon the individual and his or her relationship within the environment. His model provides the basis for understanding family’s influences on the developmental outcomes of Children The developing individual is said to be embedded in not one but several environmental systems, ranging from immediate settings, such as the family, to remote contexts, such as the broader culture. He has systematically defined the environment as a four level system: microsystem, mesosystem, exosystem, and macrosystem (see Figure 1). The microsystem consists of the immediate contexts that individuals actually experience. For most young infants the microsystem may be limited y i 5 History Time Figure 1. Bronfenbrenner’s Modiofthe Human Ecosystem (Adopted from Starver. 1994J.77). to the family. Yet this structure becomes much more complex as children mature and are exposed to day care, preschool settings, and neighborhood play areas. Not only are children likely to be influenced by the people present in their microsystems, but Children affect those people as well. For example, a very irritable or difficult infant can alienate her parents or even create friction between them that may be enough to challenge their marital relationship. Microsystems are dynamic systems in which each person influences and is influenced by the other persons present (Shaffer, 1994). Mesosystem refers to the interconnections among microsystems. According to Bronfenbrenner, Children’s development is likely to be optimized by supportive links among microsystems. For example, a toddler who has established secure emotional ties to his or her parents may be well prepared to approach and to cooperate with other children upon entering day care (Shaffer, 1994). A child’s ability to master arithmetic in elementary school may depend not only on the instruction the teacher provides but also on the extent to which such academic activities are valued and encouraged at home. The exosystem consists of settings that children never experience directly but may still affect their development. For example, children’s emotional relationships at home can also be influenced considerably by whether or not their parents enjoy their work. The type Of parents’ occupation can influence the values they hold for their children (Kohn, 1977). Macrosystem refers to as the ideology, values, beliefs, rules, and customs that dictate how children should or should not be treated, what they should be taught, and which goals they should strive for. These values differ from culture to culture and can greatly affect the kinds of experiences Children have in their homes, schools, neighborhoods, and all other contexts that directly or indirectly influence them. For example, the occurrence of Child abuse in families is much higher in those cultures (or macrosystems) that allow the use Of physical punishment and adopt a permissive attitude toward violence (Belsky, 1980). Thus macrosystems are composed of cultural environments. The cultural values, rules, and religious beliefs of the macrosystem affect children’s experience in the microsystem such as the type Of home environment and parent-Child interaction. Bronfenbrenner’s ecological approach of human development is the basis on which this study is designed. This study will examine portions of the child’s microsystem, mesosystem, exosystem. The research will investigate the interaction among these systems in affecting the academic and behavioral adjustment Of children. Belskv’s Model of the Determinants of Parenting Behavior Belsky (1984) proposes that the three factors most influential in shaping parenting behavior are the Characteristics of the child, the parent (ontogenic development, personality, and psychological resources) and the social context (marital relations, social networks, and occupational experiences) (see Figure 2). The model presumes that parenting is directly influenced by forces emanating from within the individual parent (personality), within the individual Child (Child characteristics of individuality). and from the broader social context in which the parent-child relationship is embedded- .€m.m .vmmw .>xm_om§~coamn ho 3555.050 9: .6 .305. mmoooan < .N 939”. - “5:58:00 - . - , 3:8 E25 El NOZmIOuUNLQ£U — EEO _ mczcucwn “ICE—320". I _uuth. , x3332 .225 75:23.— a , 533: .mucuEnoBSo 10 Specifically, marital relations, social networks, and occupational experiences Of parents. Furthermore, the model assumes that parents’ developmental histories, marital relations, social networks, and jobs influence individual personality and general psychological well-being Of parents and, thereby, parental functioning and, in turn, Child development (Belsky, 1984, p.84). 3. Child Characteristics There is Clear evidence that attributes of mother and child affect the nature of the interactions because the parent-child relationship involves two parties (Belsky, 1997). According to studies (Brunk & Henggeler, 1984; Anderson, Lytton, & Romney, 1986), mothers and other adults react to disobedient, negative, and/or highly active children with negative, controlling behavior. b. Parent Characteristics It has been found that adults who are psychologically healthy and mature are more likely to provide the kind of care that promotes healthy psychological development in their children (Belsky, 1990). A large amount of research indicates that mothers who are depressed tend to be less affectionate, responsive, and spontaneous with their infants and to be irritable and punitive with their older children (Fleming et al., 1988; Conger et al., 1984). It has been also found that the quality of care that parents provide is related in their own families of origin. However, a supportive relationship with a spouse or mate functions to prevent the intergenerational transmission of negative, rejecting, and insensitive maternal care (Crockenberg, 1987). c. The Man'tal Relationship Repeatedly it has been found that spousal support of both the emotional ll (e.g., love, intimacy) and instrumental (e.g., child care tasks) variety is related to enhanced parental performance (Cox et al., 1989). d. Social Network Support Mothers with more community support and less frequent negative interactions with significant others provide more positively affectionate and sensitive care to their preschoolers (Cotterell, 1986). According to Belsky (1990), social support can function in a stress- buffering manner. For example, mothers with highly irritable infants benefit most from assistance received from friends and relatives (Crockenberg & McCluskey, 1986) 9. Occupational Experience Jackson et al.’s study (1998) indicate that being employed is associated with higher educational attainment and lower scores on the measures of depressive symptoms and parental stress. All three factors (Characteristics of the child, the parent, and the social context) in the process model Of parenting will be used in this study as predictors Of maternal behavior. McLoyd’S Analytic Model of How Poverty Affects Chilggr McLoyd (1990) examines family processes affecting the socioemotional functioning Of Children living in poor families and families experiencing economic loss (Figure 3). She argues that: 12 50m .82 loads: c2920 109m am? 83 2E88w n.....lm 2.98 So: a .085. DEB/Ix .m ESE 923.30%? “cu—RAE “cask—Eek . 2.30 82.2mm . 532$ . 3239“. 38205888 . «0335) £38:— ‘ .................. Egan EEO F 5:35:30 . ages—es Eafiauaum . 23502 ESE— vocaoa . £828 a: 8&3 38m team 33 Seesaw . Eras. .Egom . magma , anoacgmn ... u. savanna 3333 . 5:333.— . nan—893 . “:2: 13 (a) poverty and economic loss diminish the capacity for supportive, consistent, and involved parenting and render parents more vulnerable to the debilitating effects of negative life events, (b) a major mediator of the link between economic hardship and parenting behavior is psychological distress deriving from an excess of negative life events, undesirable chronic conditions, and the absence and disruption of marital bond, (c) economic hardship adversely affects children’s socioemotional functioning in part through is impact on the parent’s behavior toward the child, and (d) father-child relations under conditions of economic hardship depend on the quality of relations between the mother and father. The model describes the impact of economic hardship on family processes as a function of personal Characteristics of individual family members, including Child. The model shows that parental depressed mood is a central mechanism through which economic Circumstances influence parenting behavior and, thereby, child outcomes. According to Patterson (1989), stressful experiences increase psychological distress in mothers and produce changes in family and child- management practices. McLoyd (1990) indicates that distressed mothers’ increased use of aversive, coercive discipline contributes to antisocial behavior in the child. The model is appropriate for white families although McLoyd generated the model for black families, because elements of the model are drawn from Elder’s studies of white families Of the Great Depression. McLoyd’s model is important for this study because the study focuses especially on low-income families. Garmezy’s Work on the Protective Factors of Poor Children Garmezy (1991) found certain factors that enable poor Children to circumvent life stressors. According to his work, there is strong evidence that many children and adults do overcome life’s difficulties. For example, he found that the patterns of high achievers included frequent school contact initiated by parents, the child’s exposure to stimulating, supportive school teachers, and infrequent family conflict. He disagrees that disadvantaged children are doomed to fail in their lives. He categorizes the variables which may be effective in dealing with stressful life situations into three parts: One is the modification of stressors brought about by temperament, such as activity level, reflectiveness in meeting new situations, cognitive skills, and positive responsiveness to others. Another core of variables is to be found in families in poverty that are marked by warmth, cohesion, and the presence Of some caring adult (such as a grandparent) in the absence of responsive parents or in the presence of marked marital discord. A third variable is the presence of a source of external support, as exemplified by a strong maternal substitute or a kindly concerned teacher, or the presence of an institutional structure, such as a caring agency or a church that fosters ties to a larger community (p. 421 ). A Bridging Of Concepts The theoretical framework for this study will be the bridging of concepts of Belsky’s model, McLoyd’s model, and Gannezy’s work (Figure 4). The Effects of Lack of Economic Resources on Parenting Family’s economic hardship affects maternal psychological well-being, maternal behavior, and children’s development. There is a large amount of 15 .8?on ES .esceom Leosgmm 288$. €98 SE 3568mm mOEooSo $2583.98 omco>nm 9 3___nmcmc.:> can >oco_._mom .38: .2 SHOES—NO .03”; rm .5 .EOEQOLOSOQ EEO .EOEOO_O>OO .mcozoEooOom new .mczcmcma .3966 _8_oo_oco>mn E9220 can mm___Em._ :83 :0 9522 22858 AC 83:: on... .88: .0 .> .3322 8-8 .8 .EmquaSoo EEO ..mooE 8805 < Seaman .6 £55,528 9: $8: ... imam $958 wm___Emn_ 9:85-33 Eat c2230 Co Hangman}. new 6323 .3855. Co 82285 CO Eco—2 < ”wit. 532mm 99-..»; mozmtmfimcmco mczcocmn All] _8_mo_oco>mn 2:0 36232 .9552 EOEEZER boEI 33523 M 920m“. All. mozmtouomcmco .mEoEaO_o>oo O_Eo_umo< p220 .meOEoO REDS—2 $5222 {OBOECE i_mozocoo£ .v 9:9“. 16 research that shows relationships among family’s economic hardship, maternal psychological well-being, maternal behavior, and the development of children. The lack of economic resources affects parents as individuals in a number of ways that may inhibit their healthy functioning in the parent-child relationship (Kaiser & Delaney, 1996). Living with low-income can create chronic psychological distress for individuals and rates Of depression are elevated among poor mothers (Cmic & Acevedo, 1995). According to Cmic and Acevedo (1995), chronic psychological distress becomes the psychological context from which parenting occurs. As a result of the context of psychological distress, expressions of frustration and aggression toward others increase and much of this aggression will be manifest in the context of the family and marital relationship (McLoyd, 1990). Not surprisingly, marital conflict is high among poor couples (Rubin et al., 1995). The ability to parent is weakened by the effects of the stresses associated with the lack of economic resources on psychological well-being. According to Sampson and Laub (1995), parents living with low-income evidence less capacity for supportive, consistent parenting. There is direct evidence that anxiety, depression, and irritability increase the tendency of parents to be punitive, erratic, unilateral, and less supportive of their children (McLoyd, 1990; Webster-Stratton, 1990) Garrett et al. (1994) report that the home environment of low-income families may contain fewer materials, activities, and child-inclusive routines that support children’s development. According to Sampson and Laub (1994), 17 parents in low-income families appear to be less successful in monitoring and regulating their children’s activities and behavior within the home and outside the home. Heath (1989) and Purcell-Gates (1995) describe why support for emergent literacy is lessened in families with few economic resources. They are less likely to own and use books, parents typically have lower levels Of reading Skills, and the disorganization of the home environment may lessen contexts in which reading routinely occurs. Parents most likely to be poor are female, unmarried, and members of races and ethnic groups that are the targets of discrimination (Garrett, Ng’andu, & Ferron, 1994). Parent characteristics, including education, their own experience as children, and their mental and physical health may buffer or exacerbate parents’ responses to the stresses associated with the lack of economic resources (Kaiser & Delaney, 1996). The Effects of Lack Of Economic Resources on Young Children In this part, the focus is on describing how economic hardship affects young children. The lack Of economic resources in families affects children directly and indirectly, although it is difficult to separate the direct and indirect effects of the lack of economic resources on children. According to Kaiser and Delaney (1996), the lack of economic resources affects children directly through substandard living conditions, lack Of critical material resources, inadequate nutrition, inferior health care, and fewer l8 opportunities for formal education and other development enhancing experience. The lack of economic resources also indirectly affects children through their parents and caregivers, who are simultaneously affected by the physical conditions and the cumulative psychological stresses associated with being poor. In general, the lack of economic resources has negative effects on Child health, growth, and development. Children’s academic and behavioral adjustments are considered in this part for the purposes of the study. Emergent literacy skills such as naming letters, Sightreading Simple words, and showing interest in reading materials are a bridge indicating Children’s transition into reading and writing. Children from low-income families have less frequent opportunities to learn these Skills (Sulzby, Branz, & Buhle, 1993). According to Purcell-Gates ( 1995), children from low-income families enter kindergarten with fewer of the foundational language and emergent literacy skills and have a more difficult time acquiring the basic skills of reading in the early elementary grades. Specific reading difficulties may be related to differences in vocabulary size, discourse, and narrative skills (Bloome, 1989) and familiarity with the social and cognitive expectations of the classroom context (Purcell-Gates & Dahl, 1991). The incidence Of reading-related learning difficulties is significantly higher among children from low-income families, reflecting both increased biological risks and environmental contributions (Badien, 1988; Teale, 1986). Children’s socioemotional development is also negatively affected when 19 children are raised in low-income families. Poor children present more behavioral problems at home and in school (Duncan, Brooks-Gunn, & Klebanov, 1994; McLoyd, 1990) than children who are not poor. According to McLeod and Shanahan (1993), children from low-income families exhibit elevated levels of both externalizing (e.g., acting out, aggression) and internalizing (e.g., social withdrawal, negative self-confidnece) behavior. Dodge, Pettit, and Bates (1994) identified a set of socialization patterns and social contexts that lead to poor child behavioral outcomes among children from low-income families: (a) harsh parenting, (b) lack of warmth toward the Child, (O) modeling of aggressive behavior patterns by parents, (d) differential valuing of aggression as a problem-solving strategy, (e) high frequencies Of stressful life events, (f) relative isolation and lack of social support experienced by parents, (9) limited experience with a stable group Of well-adjusted nonaggressive peers, and (h) limited cognitive stimulation and support for academic development. Mediators of Parenting in Low-Income Families The effects of lack of economic resources in families are not homogeneous across children or parents. Not all parents are affected negatively by lack of economic resources, and some portion of children raised in low- income families are resilient to risk (Garmezy, 1983). In this theoretical framework, maternal psychological well-being and maternal behavior are mediating factors between family’s economic hardship and 20 the development Of children. As described earlier, maternal psychological well- being and maternal behavior are influenced by maternal Characteristics and contextual factors. Kaiser and Delaney (1996) argue that both family and child factors mediate the effects of lack of economic resources. These factors include (a) maternal education and IQ (Bee et al., 1982); (b) social support experienced by the parent, including marital and family support and friendship support (Hashima & Amato, 1994; Rubin et al., 1994); (c) parent mental health (Webster-Stratton, 1990); (d) the family’s income relative to need (Duncan et al., 1994); (e) family Size (Bradley et al., 1994); (f) child temperament and health (Halpern, 1990); and the availability of quality child care and early intervention (Honig, 1995; Liaw & Brooks-Gunn, 1994) (p.74). According to Duncan et al. (1994), parents who have more education, higher le, fewer mental health problems, and adequate social support are most likely to use coping strategies to mediate the direct effects of lack of economic resources on their children and to provide more stimulating and supportive home environment for their children. Conceptual Map A model of the predictors of maternal behavior and adjustment of children from low-income families will be used as the conceptual model for this study (Figure 5). This model was generated by adapting Belsky’s (1984), McLoyd’s (1990), and Gannezy’s (1991) work. In this study, only young children from low- income families were included. 21 mo___Emn_ OEOOESSOIL. EC: 5530 CO EOE§=6< ccm LO_>ch.m .mEOums. CO £221“.an LO Ewe—2 < .m 9:9“. Ema; 5.5 .m om< .N 5950 .F 8338220 EEO H EoEfiafim 35323 .N Emgwafim 22.283. .9 0508.5 EEO I Y .8305 EOEOE 53353 .6 .05.. EOEco.._>cO CEO; 9.39.33 OE Co Egan—o on... £|, 3030.053.”— ..o_>m:om 35¢qu 35052 EoEcESco 362961 .0 82.8362 .m SEEM .m 85598 beam—2 .v 28 EEO .v , 889:9... .m 86?. EOESEEM .m All .92 cosmoaum .N Ego Rams. .N 5.5 SE a om< a 9303:» 2:5“. .9 mo:mt202uz0 820a...— ineEoU HES—«s. ‘lll. , 3:23 6060.05 58 £5, oocoEmom .N commence macmcmn ._. EOE: 3.85.5350 3521...: n_ m .9: 65 9:8 88:86 98 220 1 moo ._o>m_ sawmeamu 9850.2 1 ammo .....zoom EBmmE @8522 l ms. .903 EomEBE 95522 I he“? .coacoauo 9.6505. 1 ms. .55 we a em< 1 9:2 senses 52 9652: as. 25.8 £8 5:. 8:628”. 1 "SE 8:858 6.6565290 1 mo 688.3% /M.E ezz .Wyfla 491. Enos. 6.362% on... .o 939”. 101 £96; 5.3 6.2.5 1 >50 .623 6.220 1 xmwo .666 6.220 1 $5 .208 E6505 83653 $220 1 Km .ocoom coacmoom. 9.669 MEEU l ._. m we 65 merge 88596 98 2:6 1 moo ._o>o_ 87.35% 9650.2 1 ammo .ocoow BSmmE 9.6522 1 ms. .908 569.62: @5505. l h0g4. .cozcoauo mLmEos. 1 ms. .55 we... .6 em< 1 me<< 5.85.3 59 .9659: ES 9:93 £8 53. 8828a 1 $51 5258566 6.6565290 1 mo ”meeceecem .6002 3.3026 “333.3. of. N 939“. 102 (C.R.) less than i 1.0 and by adding some of the significant paths, C.R. greater than i 4.0. As a result, the statistics of the model fit were improved [GFI = .974; RMSEA = .018; x2 = 58.683 (df = 54, p = .308)]. Table 17 represents parameter estimates, CR, and model fits for the structural model and the revised structural model. The results indicated that the positive effects of grandmother’s education on mother’s education and the effects of residence with both parents until mother’s 18th birthday on mother’s age at first birth, mother’s education, and mother’s intelligence were significant at p < .05 level. The results also indicated that mother’s education has an effect on child care experience during the first three years of the child’s life and that mother’s intelligence score has an effect on neighborhood problem. Mother’s depression was affected by residence with both parents until her 18th birthday and neighborhood problems. On the other hand, the quality of the home environment was affected by mother’s intelligence, neighborhood problem, child’s sex, and child’s birth weight. The effects of child care experience during the first three years of the child’s life, the quality of the home environment, mother’s intelligence, child’ age, and child’s sex on the child’s PIAT reading recognition score were significant. The child’s behavior problem was influenced by maternal depression and the child’s age. Although the overall structural model was rejected based on the X2 test, other indices supported an adequate level of fit. The revised structural model adequately represented the causal relationships among the variables. 103 Table 17. Structural Model: Parameter Estimates Critical Ratios, and Model fits Parameters Structural Model Revised Structura_l Mod_e_l ML Estimates C.R. ML Estimates C.R. GE —> AAFB .06 1.08 .06 1.06 GE -> ME .40 7.39 .39 7.45 GE —) AFQT .42 7.97 .42 7.95 GE —) MS .04 .60 RWP —> AAFB .26 4.56 .26 4.56 RWP—aME .17 3.19 .17 3.18 RWP —) AFQT .23 4.33 .23 4.32 RWP —-> MS -.06 -1.02 -.06 -1.06 CCE (— AAFB .07 1.29 .06 1.07 CCE (— ME .37 5.78 .34 5.99 CCE (— AFQT -.07 -1.07 CCE 4— MS .09 1.55 .08 1.45 NHP (— AAFB -.10 -1.69 NHP (— ME .01 .11 NHP <— AFQT -.31 -4.93 -.33 -5.90 NHP (— MS -.06 -1.05 CESD 4— GE .09 1.30 .09 1.30 CESD <— RWP -.13 -2.14 -.13 -2.14 CESD <—AAFB .01 .13 .01 .13 CESD <— ME -.13 -1.72 -.13 -1.73 CESD (— AFQT -.07 -.99 -.07 -.99 CESD (- MS -.11 -1.94 -.11 -1.95 CESD <— CCE .11 1.71 ' .11 1.71 CESD (— NHP .16 2.60 .16 2.62 HOME (— AAFB .06 1.04 .06 1.05 HOME +— ME .03 .49 .04 .52 HOME (— AFQT .26 4.06 .26 4.05 HOME +— MS .03 .60 .03 .62 HOME (— CCE -.06 -.92 -.06 -.95 HOME (— NHP -.13 -2.27 -.13 -2.29 HOME <— CAGE -.01 -.21 HOME<—CSEX .12 2.17 .12 2.16 HOME (— CBW .20 3.74 .20 3.75 HOME 4— CESD -.05 -.83 -.05 -.80 PIAT (— CCE .11 2.01 .12 2.17 PIAT (— NHP -.03 -.57 PIAT (— HOME .14 2.45 .15 2.60 PIAT (— CAGE -.24 -4.43 -.24 -4.42 PIAT <— CSEX .18 3.26 .18 3.26 PIAT (— CBW -.00 -.05 104 Table 17 (cont'd) Parameters Structural Model Revised Structural Model ML Estimates C.R. ML Estimates C.R. PIAT <— AFQT .22 3.81 BPI <— CCE .03 .45 - BPI <— NHP .08 1.28 BPI (— HOME -.11 -1.83 -.10 -1.68 BPI (— CAGE .11 1.93 .12 2.05 BPI <— CSEX -.11 -1.93 -.11 -1.97 BPI (— CBW -.11 -1.84 -.11 -1.94 BPI (— MS -.10 -1.80 BPI (— CESD .17 3.03 12 77.17 58.68 d.f. 47 54 p .004 .308 GFI .965 .974 AGFI .912 .941 RMSEA .048 .018 Summary of Results In this section, the results of the study are summarized. The summary is presented in terms of the research hypotheses addressed in the study. Hypothesis 1: There are relationships between maternal developmental history (grandmother’s education, residence with both parents until 18th birthday) and maternal characteristics. The results presented in the earlier section are consistent with the hypothesis. The level of grandmother’s education was positively related to the 105 mother’s education level and maternal intelligence scores (see Table 2). The results also indicated that mothers who lived with both parents until her birthday tended to delay childbearing, have higher education levels and intelligence scores. The maternal developmental history variables were also related to the mother’s ethnicity. Most research in this area often disregards the importance of mothers’ own raising environments and experiences. The findings of this study suggest that it is crucial to consider the grandparents’ generation for the well- being of children in poverty because it influences children’s development directly or indirectly. Hypothesis 2: There are relationships between maternal developmental history (grandmother's education, residence with both parents until 18th birthday) and maternal depression. Hypothesis 3: There are relationships between maternal characteristics and maternal depression. Hypothesis 4: There are relationships between contextual factors and maternal depression. The hypotheses are partially supported by the results presented in the earlier section. One of the maternal developmental history variables, the mother’s residence with both parents until 18th birthday, was related to the level of maternal depression (see Table 3). Mothers who lived with both parents until their 18th birthday tended to have lower depression scores. Maternal depression was negatively related to the mother’s education level, intelligence score, and mastery score. Also, neighborhood ratings and problems were related to the mother’s level of depression. Mothers who rated the neighborhood positively and 106 reported fewer neighborhood problems tended to have lower levels of depression. Of all the predictor variables, the residence of the mother with both parents until her 18th birthday, the mother’s mastery score, and the neighborhood problem were significant predictors of maternal depression for the overall sample (see Table 7). For the married sample, marital quality was also a predictor of maternal depression, but the neighborhood problem variable was not a significant predictor. Hypothesis 5: There are relationships between maternal developmental history and the quality of the home environment mothers provide for their children. Hypothesis 6: There are relationships between maternal characteristics and the quality of the home environment mothers provide for their children. Hypothesis 7: There are relationships between child characteristics and the quality of the home environment mothers provide for their children. Hypothesis 8: There are relationships between contextual factors and the quality of the home environment mothers provide for their children. The hypotheses are partially supported by the results presented earlier. The two maternal developmental history variables were significantly related to the quality of the home environment that mothers provided for their children (see Table 4). The mother’s age at first birth, level of education, and level of intelligence were positively related to the quality of the home environment. In addition, mothers who had a spouse or a partner and who were employed provided a more supportive home environment for their children. Ethnicity also 107 was significantly related to the quality of home environment. As described earlier, it is probably because of the differences of background characteristics between African-American/Hispanic and non-Black/non-Hispanic mothers in this sample (see Table 5 and 6). For this study sample, non-Blacklnon-Hispanic mothers had more years of education, more educated mothers, and fewer neighborhood problems. Also, a larger percentage of African-American! Hispanic mothers did not live with both parents until their 18th birthday and was not living with a spouse or a partner. African-American/Hispanic mothers had lower incomes than other mothers in this poverty sample. Only one child characteristic, child’s birth weight, was positively related to the quality of the home environment. Of all the predictor variables, maternal intelligence, presence of a spouse or a partner, and child’s birth weight were significant predictors of the home environment (HOME total score) when other factors were controlled (see Table 8). Mothers’ levels of education and religiosity were related only to the HOME cognitive stimulation scores of mothers. For the married sample, age at first birth, mothers’ level of education, religiosity, marital quality, and number of children were significant predictors of the HOME total scores. It is known that a mother’s relationship with her spouse influences the quality of care provided to children (Belsky, 1990). The findings confirm Belsky’s (1997) assertion that maternal behavior is multiply determined; that is, it is unlikely that any single factor in and of itself would determine how parents cared for their children. The result is also consistent with the belief that because the parent-child relationship involves two 108 parties, attributes of each participating member affect the nature of the interactions that transpire between mother and child (Belsky, 1997). Hypothesis 9: Mothers who experience lower levels of depression will provide better quality home environments than mothers who experience higher levels of depression. The results of the zero-order correlations indicated that there was a negative relationship between the mothers’ level of depression and the quality of home environment that mothers provided for their children (see Table 4). This result is consistent with the previous study findings that parents who are more psychologically healthy tend to provide care that is more sensitive, responsive and authoritative rather than detached, intrusive or overcontrolling (Vondra & Belsky, 1993; Goodman et al., 1993; Hamish et al., 1995). However, the regression analysis indicated that the level of maternal depression was not a significant predictor of home environment when other variables were statistically controlled (see Table 8). Hypothesis 10: Children whose mothers provide more supportive home environments will demonstrate higher levels of academic achievement than children whose mothers provide lower quality of home environments. Hypothesis 11: Children whose mothers provide more supportive home environments will demonstrate lower levels of behavioral adjustment than children whose mothers provide lower quality of home environments. 109 The data were consistent with these hypotheses. There was a positive relationship between children’s reading recognition scores and the quality of the home environment that mothers provided for their children (see Table 9). Also, there was a negative relationship between children’s behavior problem scores and the quality of the home environment (see Table 9). Results of the multiple regression analyses showed that children whose mothers provided more cognitively stimulating home environment achieved higher reading recognition scores and that children whose mothers provided more supportive home environments had fewer behavior problems (see Table 10). The findings confirm Belsky’s (1997) assertion that affectively negative and unresponsive maternal behavior - that is, generally insensitive care - tends to covary with an assortment of negative developmental outcomes such as behavior problems and academic difficulty. Hypothesis 12: There are relationships between maternal developmental history and children’s levels of academic achievement, when the quality of the home environment is controlled. Hypothesis 13: There are relationships between maternal characteristics and children’s levels of academic achievement, when the quality of the home environment is controlled. Hypothesis 14: There are relationships between child characteristics and children’s levels of academic achievement, when the quality of the home environment is controlled. Hypothesis 15: There are relationships between contextual factors and children’s levels of academic achievement, when the quality of the home environment is controlled. 110 Hypothesis 12 was not supported by the data. The two maternal developmental history variables were unrelated to the children’s reading recognition scores when the quality of the home environment (cognitive stimulation) was controlled (see Table 10). It means that the grandmother’s education had an indirect effect on the child’s academic achievement because there was a significant positive relationship between the grandmother’s education and the child’s academic achievement (see Table 9). The other hypotheses were partially supported by the data. Maternal intelligence, regular child care experience during the first three years of the child’s life, child’s age, and child’s sex were significant predictors of the child’s reading recognition score when the quality of home environment was controlled. This finding indicated that these four variables had direct and indirect effects on the child’s academic achievement. On the other hand, mother’s age at first birth, mother’s education, ethnicity, number of children, and neighborhood problems had only indirect influence on the child’s academic achievement via the home environment that the mother provided for her child (see Table 9 and 10). Hypothesis 16: There are relationships between maternal developmental history and children's levels of behavioral adjustment, when the quality of the home environment is controlled. Hypothesis 17: There are relationships between maternal characteristics and children’s levels of behavioral adjustment, when the quality of the home environment is controlled. Hypothesis 18: There are relationships between child characteristics and children’s levels of behavioral adjustment, when the quality of the home environment is controlled. 111 Hypothesis 19: There are relationships between contextual factors and children’s levels of behavioral adjustment, when the quality of the home environment is controlled. Hypothesis 16 and hypothesis 18 were not supported by the data. The two maternal developmental history variables and the three child characteristic variables were unrelated to the children’s behavior problem scores when the quality of the home environment was controlled (see Table 10). The finding indicates that the mother’s mastery score and the three child characteristics did not have direct effects on the child’s behavior problem. Hypothesis 17 and 19 were partially supported by the data. The results showed that the presence of a spouse or a partner, and the number of children were significant predictors of the child’s behavioral adjustment when the quality of the home environment was statistically controlled. Hypothesis 20: There is a relationship between maternal depression level and children’s levels of academic achievement, when the quality of the home environment is controlled. Hypothesis 21: There is a relationship between maternal depression level and children’s levels of behavioral adjustment, when the quality of the home environment is controlled. Although Hypothesis 21 was supported by the data, Hypothesis 20 was not supported by the data. The results showed that maternal depression was a significant predictor only for the child’s behavioral adjustment not for the child’s academic achievement (see Table 10). The results of this study indicated that 112 1".— maternal psychological well-being had a direct and an indirect influence on the child’s behavioral adjustment (see Table 9 and 10). Although Coiro’s study (1997) showed that mothers’ depression scores predicted more behavior problems and lower school readiness, her study also indicated that depressive symptoms were much more strongly related to behavior problems than to school readiness. According to Coiro (1997), parenting that is more supportive and stimulating partially mediates the association between mothers’ depressive symptoms and children’s behavior problems. The result of this study supports this finding. Hypothesis 22: There is a difference between the most successful low-income children (scoring in the top quartile for the sample on PIAT reading recognition and scoring in the bottom quartile for the sample on BPI) and the least successful low-income children (scoring in the bottom quartile for the sample on PIAT reading recognition and scoring in the top quartile for the sample on BPI) in child characteristics. Hypothesis 23: There is a difference between the most successful low-income children and the least successful low-income children in maternal developmental history, characteristics, and behavior. Hypothesis 24: There is a difference between the most successful low-income children and the least successful low-income children in contextual factors. For the child’s academic achievement (PIAT), there were significant ' differences between the most successful low-income children and the least successful low-income children in terms of child's age, child' sex, grandmother’s education level, ethnicity, maternal intelligence score, and the quality of home environment (see Table 11, 12, and 15). On the other hand, for the child’s behavioral adjustment (BPI), child’s birth weight, maternal depression, and the 113 cognitively stimulating home environment for the child were significantly different between the most successful low-income children and the least successful low- income children (see Table 13, 14, and 16). The results are consistent with the previous study finding that the most successful and the least successful children from low-income families received very different caregiving and lived in different contexts (Luster et al., 2000). 114 Chapter 5 SUMMARY, CONCLUSIONS, LIMITATIONS, IMPLICATIONS, AND SUGGESTIONS FOR FUTURE RESEARCH This chapter presents a summary of the study, conclusions, limitations, implications, and suggestions for future research. I Summary of the Study I‘- The major purposes of this study were to investigate some predictors of maternal psychological well-being and the quality of home environment that mothers provide for their children, to identify factors that predict individual differences in academic and behavioral adjustments of 5 to 8 year-old children from low-income families, and to examine a conceptual model of predictors of maternal behavior and the adjustment of children from low-income families using the NLSY data. In this section, the purposes and findings of this study pertaining to the objectives of the study are summarized. Objective 1 The first objective was to determine what maternal characteristics and contextual factors are associated with maternal psychological well-being. Results of the study show that for the overall sample, the mother’s residence with parents until her 18th birthday, mastery, and neighborhood problems were predictive of her psychological well-being. In other words, mothers who lived 115 with both parents until their 18th birthday and mothers with higher mastery scores showed higher levels of psychological well-being. The results indicated that mothers who reported fewer neighborhood problems showed higher level of psychological well-being. For the sample of those who were married, their response to questions about marital quality was one of the significant predictors of maternal psychological well-being. That is, mothers with better marital quality showed higher psychological well-being. Again, mothers who lived with both parents until their 18th birthday and mothers with higher mastery scores showed higher psychological well-being for the married group. The relationships were stronger than those of the overall sample. Objective 2 The second objective was to determine what child characteristics, maternal characteristics, and contextual factors are associated with quality of the home environment. The results of the analysis indicated that mothers’ higher levels of intelligence, the presence of a spouse or partner living in the home, fewer children, and heavier birth weight children were predictive of higher HOME (Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment) scores in the overall sample. Objective 3 The third objective was to determine if maternal psychological well-being is related to the quality of the home environment that mothers provide. The result showed a positive relationship between maternal psychological well-being 116 and the quality of the home environment mothers provide. In other words, mothers with higher levels of psychological well-being tended to provide more supportive home environments for their children. However, maternal psychological well-being was not a significant predictor of the quality of the home environment mothers provide according to the regression analyses. Objective 4 The fourth objective of this study was to determine if the quality of the home environment is related to children’s academic and behavioral adjustment. The result of the analyses showed that there were relationships between the quality of the home environment and children’s academic achievement and behavioral adjustment. Children whose mothers provided a more supportive home environment had higher reading recognition scores and lower behavioral problem scores. ‘ Objective 5 The fifth objective was to determine the relationships between the predictor variables (child characteristics, maternal characteristics, contextual factors, and maternal psychological well-being) and children’s academic and behavioral adjustment, when the quality of the home environment is statistically controlled. For the children’s reading recognition scores, mothers’ intelligence scores, regular child care experience during the first three years of the children’s lives, child’s age and sex were significant predictors. In other words, children who had mothers with higher intelligence scores and children who had more years of regular child care experience during the first three years of their lives 117 showed higher reading recognition scores when the quality of the home environment was statistically controlled. Younger children and girls showed higher reading recognition scores. Maternal psychological well-being, the presence of a spouse or a partner, and the number of children in the household were significant predictors of the children’s behavior problems when the quality of the home environment was statistically controlled. Children who have psychologically healthy mothers and more siblings showed fewer behavior z-fiv problems. Contrary to most research, children who lived with their mothers’ spouse or partner showed more behavior problems. Probably the spouse or partner was not a supportive figure for the child in this sample. However, as described earlier in the result section, the child living with his or her father was not significantly related to child’s behavior problems. One finding of this study indicated that children who have mothers with better marital quality showed fewer behavior problems. Objective 6 The final objective of this study was to identify what factors contribute to successful outcomes, in terms of academic achievement and behavioral adjustment of young children from low-income families. According to the result of the analyses, academically successful children were relatively young and female. Academically successful children had grandmothers who were more educated and had mothers with higher intelligence scores. Their mothers also provided a more supportive home environment. The result also indicated that a larger percentage of non-Black/non-Hispanic children was academically successful as 118 compared to African-AmericanIHISpanic children. As described in the earlier chapter, the two groups of this study sample showed significantly different background characteristics. In this study sample, African-American/Hispanic mothers had lower levels of education and their own mothers also had fewer years of education. A larger percentage of African-American/Hispanic mothers did not live with both parents until their 18th birthday and were not living with a spouse or a partner than other mothers. These differences must be taken into ‘ consideration as to why a larger percentage of African-American/Hispanic children were in the group of academically least successful children. Wm. . Successful children in terms of behavioral adjustment tended to have heavier birth weight, less depressed mothers, and a more cognitively stimulating home environment. There was no significant difference between the most successful and the least successful children in terms of behavioral adjustment on ethnicity. Ethnicity was not a significant predictor of maternal psychological well- being and the quality of the home environment. Conclusions Findings from this study are consistent with Bronfenbrenner’s ecological model of human development. Bronfenbrenner (1979) emphasizes the importance of the family and other microsystems as the context in which the child develops. The child develops in relationships within various kinds of environments. For example, findings of this study confirm that the child’s family, 119 neighborhood, as well as child care setting influence the developmental outcomes of the child. The proposed model (see Figure 5) was tested partially and wholly by the analyses of selected NLSY data. Although it was difficult to draw latent variables from the data by grouping with limited variables, the proposed model was meaningful according to SEM analyses (see Figure 6). In addition, the revised structural model adequately represented the causal relationships among the variables (see Figure 7). This study extends a theoretical model in which multiple factors influence the adjustments of the children from low-income families. The findings of this study extend previous research on relationships among family factors, maternal well-being, maternal parenting behavior, and the development of young children by examining the effects of relatively wide range of predictors on relationships between maternal well-being and maternal parenting behavior, and their direct effects on the adjustments of children in poverty. One of the major findings of this study is that there was a positive effect of the grandmother’s education on the mother’s education, and the residence with both parents until the mother’s 18th birthday was positively related to the mother’s age at first birth, the mother’s education and intelligence. The findings suggest that the grandmother’s education and the mother's own family experience have indirect influences on the developmental outcomes of the child. According to the results of this study, the mother’s age at first birth, the mother’s education and intelligence were positively related to the quality of the home environment that the mother provided for her child. Also it was found that the mother’s age at first 120 birth, the mother’s education, the mother’s intelligence, and the quality of the home environment were positively related to the child’s academic achievement, and the quality of the home environment was negatively related to the child’s behavior problem. The findings of this study also confirm the importance of early intervention programs like regular child care for a favorable outcome of children from low- income families. The results indicated that regular child care experience during the first three years of the child’s life had positive influence on the child’s academic achievement. Previous studies indicate that enhancing the cognitive and social competence in children and changing patterns of interactions in the family through early intervention programs can have long-term cumulative protective effects, resulting in prevention of antisocial behavior and delinquency (Yoshikawa, 1994; Zigler et al., 1992). Therefore, it is very important to provide quality and affordable child care programs for promoting healthy development of low-income children. Another major finding of this study is that maternal psychological well- being was affected by the mother’s residence with both parents until her 18th birthday and by her perception of neighborhood problems. It shows how important the mother’s own family experience was for her well-being. In addition, it shows that it is imperative to consider the neighborhood environment for the mother’s well-being and the healthy development of low-income children. The neighborhood environment also had an influence on the quality of the home environment that mothers provided for their children. The results of this study 121 are consistent with previous research findings about the neighborhood environment. That is, the neighborhood environment influences the development of the child directly and indirectly (Chase-Lansdale et al., 1997). Results from this study were also consistent with the assumption that parental behavior influences child development. Children who achieved higher scores on the academic achievement measures had mothers who provided more cognitively stimulating home environments. Also, children who had fewer behavioral problems had mothers who provided more supportive home environments. It is known that the combination of warm and structured child- rearing practices in parents with reasonably high expectations for the competence of their children is strongly tied to success in multiple domains and resilience among children at risk (Masten 8 Coatsworth, 1998). The results of this study support Belsky’s (1984) assertion that the quality of the home environment mothers provide for their children is influenced by multiple factors, such as child characteristics, maternal characteristics, and contextual factors. In this study, the mother’s intelligence, the mother’s age at first birth, the mother’s education, the mother’s religiosity, the presence of a spouse or a partner, the mother’s marital quality, the number of children in the household, and child’s birth weight were the predictors of the quality of the home environment that mothers provided for their children. Although maternal psychological well-being was not a significant predictor of the quality of the home environment mothers provide, the present study showed the direct effect of maternal psychological well-being on the child’s 122 behavioral adjustment. The result is somewhat supported by the findings of Coiro’s study (1997) that mothers’ depression scores predicted more behavior problems and lower school readiness, and depressive symptoms were much more strongly related to behavior problems than to school readiness. For the overall study sample, the family factors that significantly related to the academically successful children were the grandmother’s education, the mother’s intelligence, and the quality of the home environment. Maternal psychological well-being and the quality of the home environment were the family factors that made a difference between the most successful and the least successful children in terms of the behavioral adjustment. Additional analyses indicated that children who had more regular child care experience as well as a supportive home environment were academically successful, and children who had mothers with higher mastery scores and lower depression scores were successful in terms of behavioral adjustment among the children from single parent families. For the African-American1Hispanic sub-sample, the academically successful children had mothers with higher ages at the start of first pregnancy in addition to the three variables (grandmother’s education, mother’s intelligence, the quality of the home environment). Children with the mothers who had a higher level of psychological well-being showed significantly lower behavior problems in this sample. The results are consistent with of the previous research findings on the characteristics of resilient children (Garmezy, 1991; Werner, 1992). 123 Limitations Although the proposed model adequately represented the causal relationships among the variables, several other variables that would have been useful to test a more complete model were not available in the NLSY data set. For example, there was no data on social support networks for the mothers. In addition, variables of child characteristics were limited. Despite the range of variables included this study, additional measures of contextual factors would have strengthened the study further. The effects of school environments, peer relationships, and sibling interactions on the development of the children were not examined in this study. Since the short-form of the HOME Inventory was used in this study instead of the complete version, the relations between the predictor variables and the quality of the home environment mothers provided may have been attenuated. Likewise, the relationship between the HOME and the child outcomes may have been underestimated. Although this study included only poor (incomes below poverty line) and near poor children (incomes between 100% and 185% of the poverty line), it was difficult to exclude the income effect in this group. Even though the family income was not a significant predictor of the quality of the home environment and children’s adjustments, there was a positive relationship between family income and the quality of home environment, and there was a negative relationship between the family income and the maternal depression within this poverty 124 sample. It was impossible to completely exclude the income effect on other variables in this study. In this study, maternal psychological well-being was measured only by the maternal depression score (CESD). Although maternal depression is one of the representative components of maternal psychological well-being, including other variables such as maternal happiness or satisfaction with her life would be helpful for more understanding of maternal psychological well-being. If it were possible to get more integrative measures of maternal psychological well-being, the relationships between maternal psychological well-being and other variables would be more clearly explained. Only maternal variables were used in this study. Examining paternal variables, such as father's characteristics, paternal psychological well-being, and the father’s relationship with his child would enhance the understanding of the developmental outcomes of children. Implications The results of this study confirm the findings of previous studies that parenting behavior is multiply determined and parenting behavior is related to children’s outcomes. The findings imply that it is important to take an ecological perspective in working with families. That is, one should consider parents’ developmental histories and characteristics of parents, the child, and the context for making a difference in the child’s developmental outcomes. This approach 125 will be effective for changing parenting behavior and improving family functioning. In addition, the family systems perspective, which complements the ecological approach, views the family as an organized system composed of interdependent relationships or subsystems (Chase-Lansdale et al., 1992). Roles in these subsystems such as parental, sibling, spousal, and extra familial change over time and with different circumstances. Adoption of the basic tenets of the ecological and family systems perspectives is crucial to an understanding of how best to intervene to promote the optimal development of children. Several findings of this study suggest implications for child and family policy for low-income families. The result of this study indicated that child care experience during the first three years of the child’s life had a positive effect on the academic achievement of poor child. Data showed that child care, especially in the early years of life, is very important for enhancing the healthy development of the poor child. Child and family policy makers should pay attention to the findings and ensure that poor children have an opportunity to get child care experience. Through quality child care, children are exposed in supportive environments, and parents of the children may get some useful information about parenting and educating their children. The findings of this studyalso indicated that multigenerational programs are needed for the optimal development of children in poverty. Not only maternal characteristics influenced the adjustment of the children, but also grandmothers’ characteristics such as education level had an indirect effect on the adjustment of the children. Programs that only focus on the children may not be effective since it has been argued that disadvantaged children and families need a more intensive and encompassing treatment. There is a need for programs that aim to increase the participation of mothers and children in early childhood education, parenting education, adult education, and job training. Furthermore, services which broadly focus on the family as a whole, rather than just on mothers or just on children will be more effective. It may be more effective if the services for families begin as early as possible in the life of the child. This study also indicated that it is important to support parents not only physically but also psychologically for the healthy development of the children. In this study, several predictors of maternal psychological well-being were found. Parent educators and service providers should keep in mind the research findings so that they may support the low-income families more effectively. In addition, one should not disregard the effects of the neighborhood environment on the development of children. The result of this study indicated that the development of children is indirectly influenced by the neighborhood environment. Serious neighborhood problems are associated with maternal depression and maternal depression has a negative influence on parenting behavior and the child’s developmental outcome. Therefore, it is important to make some plans for improving the neighborhood environment for the optimal development of the poor child. It is evident that parents should use their influence to initiate-educational activities in the household and regularly monitor the young child’s use of time and space for the sake of the child’s healthy development. It is also important that 127 parents clearly define and fully accept their responsibilities for parenting. That is parents should be a provider, a teacher, a guide, and a role model for their children. Basically, the economic problems must be solved through the opportunity structure of larger society. The problems of the low-income families, even those fostering children’s developmental outcomes, are not simply a consequence of family members’ behavior. Limited access to needed resources plays a considerable part (Clark, 1983). Suggestions for Future Research Much of the previous research on children living in poverty has focused on identifying the negative consequences of poverty on children and finding risk factors of poor children and their families. Researchers have tended to overlook the positive aspects of family life among low-income families. However, recently many researchers have become aware of the importance of studying why many poor children are successful in school and are well adjusted. If we learn what makes the children different from other poor children, it would be beneficial for not only poor children and their families, but also intervention program providers and policy makers. More research is needed to fully understand what factors influence the psychological well-being of low-income parents and how the parental psychological well-being is related to the quality of the home environment 128 parents provide for their children, and the developmental outcomes of the children. Other predictor variables such as a social support network, parents’ expectations and aspirations for the child, levels of tension or cohesion in the family, housing quality, and parents’ health status need to be investigated. The effect of parental religiosity on the parental psychological well-being and on the parental behavior is also meaningful to study although the present study could not investigate these relationships in depth because of the lack of the information on religiosity. In addition, it is important to examine the relationships between other family members such as fathers, siblings, grandparents and the child. It will be interesting to study whether those relationships influence the developmental outcomes of the child. Garmezy (1991) categorized possibly effective variables in dealing with stressful life situations into three parts: individual, family, and social support factors. Individual factors such as child’s temperament, personality, intelligence, physical distinctiveness, self-regulation, and gender have not been examined very well. Many studies have focused on identifying risk and protective factors implicated in poor children’s academic achievement and behavioral adjustment. While it is important to identify those factors, it is more important to understand how risk and protective factors affect children's developmental outcomes. Good empirical research is needed to document the specific effects of particular family interaction processes such as sibling recreational interaction or the pedagogic 129 effects of specific television and radio programming on children’s academic achievement and behavioral adjustment. One interesting variable is the parental role model, that is, whether children select one of their parents as their most influential role model. This variable is valuable to study more because it may provide children’s own overall appreciation on their parents’ characters, behaviors, and values. This factor is different from variables such as parental education, occupation, or home 131 environment which is prejudged by other people’s standards and values. It is also valuable to investigate whether children meet a supportive mentor during his or her life. The supportive mentor can be not only their mother or father but also a teacher, a friend, a pastor, a grandmother, a grandfather, a relative, or a service provider. One continuously supportive figure in someone’s life may have the great power to change his/her life or to help him/her overcoming his obstacles. Future studies may focus on examining the effect of different cultures and ethnicities to find some differences and similarities on parenting behavior, parental belief system, and developmental outcomes of the child. It may be meaningful to investigate unique child rearing systems of different cultures and the contribution of them on the child's developmental outcomes. Increased knowledge of the home functioning patterns in different ethnic communities will enhance the prospect of developing appropriate family and child policies. Researchers should avoid their biased understanding of the target population if they want to produce true knowledge for serving the target 130 population. Researchers often misjudge other people’s world according to their preoccupied norms or standards. There might be a gap between researchers and subjects in perceiving or interpreting the same life situation. Researchers should put their efforts on understanding a target population’s own perceptions of risk and protective factors. One possible solution for this task is using a qualitative research method such as a case study or a focus group. Sound qualitative studies on the issue can provide proper directions for quantitative studies. More conceptual or theoretical models that explain normative child development in the context of poverty are needed to guide research on individual differences in achievement and adjustment of children from low-income families. Also, there is a need for developing relatively limited conceptual models that fit into specific research areas such as a particular ethnic group, an adolescent mother group, or familial factors as mediators of links between poverty and children’s developmental outcomes. Many studies have examined the effects of home environment on the developmental outcomes of children from low-income families. However, future research should focus on investigating the effects of the quality of the home environment according to children’s ages. There might be a different amount of influence of the home environment on poor children’s outcomes with different ages of children. Longitudinal studies on this topic will be beneficial for examining the developmental pathways of the child. The NLSY data set provides 131 an opportunity to study the changes on maternal parenting behavior, child’s outcome, and the family’s living environments. Less work has been conducted on the effects of extra-familial or community-level forces on the development of poor children. A growing body of research indicates that living in disorganized, socially isolated (few adults who are employed, a large number of single mothers, and few opportunities for work), and resource-poor neighborhoods may have negative influences on children and their parents. It may not be possible to find all the factors that make individual differences among poor children and to identify the processes underlying between these factors and children’s outcomes; the children are different from birth and they live in diverse and complex environments. However, researchers should continuously put their efforts toward identifying developmental pathways of poor children for increasing opportunities to better their lives. 132 APPENDIX A 133 APPENDIX A A Measure of the Marital Quality R49580. Frequency R 8 Husband/Partner Calmly Discuss Something (Hand Card) How often do you and your (husband/partner) calmly discuss something...Almost every day, once or twice a week, once or twice a month or less than once a month? 1 Less than once a month 2 Once or twice a month 3 Once or twice a week 4 Almost every day R49581. Frequency R & Husband/Laugh Together (Hand Card) How often do you and your (husband/partner) laugh together something...Almost every day, once or twice a week, once or twice a month or less than once a month? 1 Less than once a month 2 Once or twice a month 3 Once or twice a week 4 Almost every day R49582. Frequency R 8 Husband/Partner Tell Each Other About Day (Hand Card) How often do you and your (husband/partner) tell each other about your day. . .Almost every day, once or twice a week, once or twice a month or less than once a month? 1 Less than once a month 2 Once or twice a month 3 Once or twice a week 4 Almost every day R49583. Frequency R 8 Husband/Partner Argue About-Chores 8 Responsibilities (Hand Card) How frequently do you and your (husband/partner) have arguments about chores and responsibilities...often, sometimes, hardly ever, or never? 1 Often 2 Sometimes 3 Hardly ever 4 Never R49584. Frequency R 8 Husband/Partner Argue About-Children (Hand Card) How frequently do you and your (husband/partner) have arguments about your children. ..often, sometimes, hardly ever, or never? 1 Often 2 Sometimes 134 3 Hardly ever 4 Never R49585. Frequency R 8 Husband/Partner Argue About-Money (Hand Card) How frequently do you and your (husband/partner) have arguments about money...often, sometimes, hardly ever, or never? 1 Often 2 Sometimes 3 Hardly ever 4 Never R49586. Frequency R 8 Husband/Partner Argue About-Showing Affection (Hand Card) How frequently do you and your (husband/partner) have arguments about showing affection to each other. . . often, sometimes, hardly ever, or never? 1 Often 2 Sometimes 3 Hardly ever 4 Never R49587. Frequency R 8 Husband/Partner Argue About-Religion (Hand Card) How frequently do you and your (husband/partner) have arguments about religion...often, sometimes, hardly ever, or never? 1 Often 2 Sometimes 3 Hardly ever 4 Never R49588. Frequency R 8 Husband/Partner Argue About-Leisure Time (Hand Card) How frequently do you and your (husband/partner) have arguments about leisure or free time. . . often, sometimes, hardly ever, or never? 1 Often 2 Sometimes 3 Hardly ever 4 Never R49589. Frequency R 8 Husband/Partner Argue About-Drinking (Hand Card) How frequently do you and your (husband/partner) have arguments about drinking...often, sometimes, hardly ever, or never? 1 Often 2 Sometimes 3 Hardly ever 4 Never R49590. Frequency R 8 Husband/Partner Argue About-Other Women (Hand Card) How frequently do you and your (husband/partner) have arguments about other women...often, sometimes, hardly ever, or never? 135 1 Often 2 Sometimes 3 Hardly ever 4 Never R49591. Frequency R 8 Husband/Partner Argue About-His Relatives (Hand Card) How frequently do you and your (husband/partner) have arguments about his relatives...often, sometimes, hardly ever, or never? 1 Often 2 Sometimes 3 Hardly ever 4 Never R49592. Frequency R 8 Husband/Partner Argue About-Your Relatives (Hand Card) How frequently do you and your (husband/partner) have arguments about your relatives. . . often, sometimes, hardly ever, or never? 1 Often 2 Sometimes 3 Hardly ever 4 Never 136 APPENDIX B 137 APPENDIX B A Measure of Maternal Psychological Well-Being A short form of the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression (CESD) scale was used for the 1994 interview. (Hand Card) Now I am going to read a list of the ways that you might have felt or behaves recently. After each statement, please tell me how often you felt this way during the past week. R49783. CESD — Poor Appetite (Hand Card) During the past week...l did not feel like eating; My appetite was poor (Interviewer: If necessary, remind Respondentz) Please tell me how often you felt this way during the past week. 0 Rarely/None of the Time/1 Day 1 Some/A Little of the Time/1-2 Days 2 Occasionally/Moderate Amount of the Timel3-4 Days 3 Most/All of the Time/5-7 Days R49784. CESD — Trouble Keeping Mind on Tasks (Hand Card) During the past week...l had trouble keeping my mind on what I was doing. (Interviewer: If necessary, remind Respondent) Please tell me how often you felt this way during the past week. 0 Rarely/None of the Time/1 Day 1 Some/A Little of the Time/1-2 Days 2 Occasionally/Moderate Amount of the Time/3-4 Days 3 Most/All of the Time/5-7 Days R49785. CESD - Depressed (Hand Card) During the past week...l felt depressed. (Interviewer: If necessary, remind Respondent) Please tell me how often you felt this way during the past week. 0 Rarely/None of the Time/1 Day 1 Some/A Little of the Time/1-2 Days 2 Occasionally/Moderate Amount of the Timel3-4 Days 3 Most/All of the Time/5-7 Days R49786. CESD - Everything Took Extra Effort (Hand Card) During the past week...l felt that everything I did was an effort. (Interviewer: If necessary, remind Respondentz) Please tell me how often you felt this way during the past week. 138 Rarely/None of the Time/1 Day Some/A Little of the Time/1-2 Days Occasionally/Moderate Amount of the Time/3-4 Days Most/All of the Time/5-7 Days (JON—*0 R49787. CESD - Restless Sleep (Hand Card) During the past week...My sleep was restless. (Interviewer: If necessary, remind Respondent) Please tell me how often you felt this way during the past week. 0 Rarely/None of the Time/1 Day 1 Some/A Little of the Time/1-2 Days 2 Occasionally/Moderate Amount of the Time/3-4 Days 3 Most/All of the Time/5-7 Days R49788. CESD - Sad (Hand Card) During the past week...l felt sad. (Interviewer: If necessary, remind Respondent) Please tell me how often you felt this way during the past week. 0 Rarely/None of the Time/1 Day 1 Some/A Little of the Time/1-2 Days 2 Occasionally/Moderate Amount of the Time/3-4 Days 3 Most/All of the Time/5-7 Days R49789. CESD — Could Not Get Going (Hand Card) During the past week...l could not get “going”. (Interviewer: If necessary, remind Respondent) Please tell me how often you felt this way during the past week. 0 Rarely/None of the Time/1 Day 1 Some/A Little of the Time/1-2 Days 2 Occasionally/Moderate Amount of the Timel3-4 Days 3 Most/All of the Time/5-7 Days 139 APPENDIX C 140 APPENDIX C Measures of the Home Environment The number of the home environment items correspond to the number of the items in the original measures, as presented in the 1984 Home Observation Measurement of the Environment (HOME) manual. Items marked with an asterisk were items that were not included on the original versions of the HOME but were added to the Home Observation Measurement of the Environment- Short Form (HOME-SF) for the National Longitudinal Survey of Youth. The subscales which the items were taken are included. HOME-SF (Preschool) Learning Stimulation 3. Child has record player and at least five children’s records. 7. Child has at least 10 children’s books. 10. Family subscribes to at least one magazine. 11. Child is encouraged to Ieam shapes. Language Stimulation 13. Child is encouraged to learn the alphabet. 17. Parent’s voice conveys positive feeling to child. 18. Child is permitted choice in breakfast or lunch menu. Physical Environment 20. Play environment appears safe. (This item combines 19 and 20 from original HOME: Building appears safe (19), and outside play environment appears safe (20).) 21. Interior of apartment is not dark or perceptually monotonous. 25. House is reasonably clean. (in the original HOME the item is: House is reasonably clean and minimally cluttered. This item was divided into two items for the HOME-SF.) Warmth and Acceptance 27. Parent converses with child at least twice during visit. 28. Parent answers child’s questions or requests verbally. 31. Parent caresses, kisses, or cuddles child during visit. Academic Stimulation 32. Child is encouraged to Ieam colors. 36. Child is encouraged to learn numbers. 141 Modeling 39. TV is used judiciously. 40. Parent introduces visitor to child. 41. Child can hit parent without harsh reprisal. Variety in Experience 44. Child is taken on outing by family member at least every other week. _ 45. Child has been taken to a museum during past year. 50. Child eats at least one meal per day with mother and father. Acceptance 53. Parent does not use physical restraint during visit. 54. Parent neither slaps nor spanks child during visit. 55. No more than one instance of physical punishment during past week. * About how often do you read stories to your child? (This item was developed by the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development for inclusion in the HOME-SF). * All rooms in the house are reasonably free of clutter (see number 25). HOME-SF (Elementary School) Emotional and Vertgl Responsivitv 5. Parent encourages child to contribute to the conversation during visit. 7. Parent responds to child’s questions during interview. 9. Parent’s voice conveys positive feelings about child. Encouragement of Maturity 11. Family requires child to carry out certain selfcare routines, e.g., makes beds, cleans rooms, cleans up after spill, bathes self. 12. Family requires child to keep living and play area reasonably clean and straight. 15. Parent introduces interviewer to child. Emotional Climate 19. Mother reports no more than one instance of physical punishment occurred during past week (“past month” in the original version of the HOME). 20. Child can express negative feelings toward parent without harsh reprisals. 23. Parent talks to child during visit (beyond correction and introduction). Growth Fostering Materials and Experiences 27. Child has free access to musical instrument (piano, drum, ukulele, or guitar, etc.) 29. Child has free access to at least ten appropriate books. 30. Parent buys and reads a newspaper daily. 142 s’hhf “18 .'\’~ 1.4-3!: 1 Provision for Active Stimulation 35. Family encourages child to develop and sustain hobbies. 37. Family provides lessons or organizational membership to support child’s talents (especially Y membership, gymnastic lessons, art center, etc.) 40. Family member has taken child, or arranged for child to go to a scientific, historical or art museum within the past year. fiaalilv Particiiation in Developmentally Stimulating Experiences 42. Family visits or receives visits form relatives or friends at least once every other week. 44. Family member has taken child, or arranged for child to attend some type of 5"; live musical or theatre performance. 7’ 46. Parents discuss television programs with child. Pientalalnvolvement 48. Father (or father substitute) regularly engages in outdoor recreation with child. 49. Child eats and spends some time with father or father figure, 4 days a week. . 50. Child eats at least one meal per day, on most days, with mother and father (or mother and father figures). Aspects of the Physical Environment 53. The interior of the apartment is not dark or perceptually monotonous. 55. All visible rooms of the house are reasonably clean. (In the original HOME, the item is: All visible rooms in the house are reasonably clean and minimally cluttered. This item was divided into two items for the HOME-SF). 58. Building has no potentially dangerous structural or health defects (e.g., plaster coming down from ceiling, stairways with boards missing, rodents, etc.). * About how often do you read stories to your child? (This item and the next item were developed by the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development for inclusion in the HOME-SF). * About how often does your child read for enjoyment? * All visible rooms in the house are minimally cluttered (see item 55). 143 APPENDIX D 144 APPENDIX D A Measure of the Neighborhood Environment Neighborhood Rating R49598. R's Rating of Neighborhood for Raising Children How would you rate your neighborhood as a place to raise children? Would tou say it is excellent, very good, good, fair, or poor? 1 Poor 2 Fair 3 Good 4 Very Good 5 Excellent Neighborhoodiroblems I am going to read a list of problems that neighborhoods sometimes have. For each one please tell me if it is a big problem in your won neighborhood, somewhat of a problem or not a problem at all. R49600. Neighborhood Problems - People Don’t Respect Rules and Laws Enough (Hand Card)... People don’t have enough respect for rules and laws. (Interviewer: If necessary remind respondent) Is this a big problem in your own neighborhood, somewhat of a problem or not a problem at all? 1 Not a problem 2 Somewhat of a problem 3 Big problem R49601. Neighborhood Problems - Crime and Violence (Hand Card)...Crime and violence (Interviewer: If necessary remind respondent) Is this a big problem in your own neighborhood, somewhat of a problem or not a problem at all? 1 Not a problem 2 Somewhat of a problem 3 Big problem R49602. Neighborhood Problems - Abandoned or Run-Down Buildings (Hand Card)...Abandoned or run-down buildings (Interviewer: If necessary remind respondent) Is this a big problem in your own neighborhood, somewhat of a problem or not a problem at all? 1 Not a problem 2 Somewhat of a problem 145 3 Big problem R49603. Neighborhood Problems - Not Enough Police Protection (Hand Card)... Not enough police protection. (Interviewer: If necessary remind respondent) Is this a big problem in your own neighborhood, somewhat of a problem or not a problem at all? 1 Not a problem 2 Somewhat of a problem 3 Big problem R49604. Neighborhood Problems - Not Enough Public Transportation (Hand Card)... Not enough public transportation (Interviewer: If necessary remind respondent) Is this a big problem in your own neighborhood, somewhat of a problem or not a problem at all? 1 Not a problem 2 Somewhat of a problem 3 Big problem R49605. Neighborhood Problems — Too Many Parents Who Don’t Supervise Their Children (Hand Card)...Too many parents who don’t supervise their children. (Interviewer: If necessary remind respondent) Is this a big problem in your own neighborhood, somewhat of a problem or not a problem at all? 1 Not a problem 2 Somewhat of a problem 3 Big problem R49606. Neighborhood Problems - People Keep to Themselves, Don’t Care About Neighborhood (Hand Card)... People keep to themselves and don’t care what goes on in the neighborhood. : (Interviewer: If necessary remind respondent) Is this a big problem in your own neighborhood, somewhat of a problem or not a problem at all? 1 Not a problem 2 Somewhat of a problem 3 Big problem R49607. Neighborhood Problems - Lots of People Who Can’t Find Jobs (Hand Card)... Lots of people who can’t find jobs. (Interviewer: If necessary remind respondent) Is this a big problem in your own neighborhood, somewhat of a problem or not a problem at all? 1 Not a problem 2 Somewhat of a problem 3 Big problem 146 BIBLIOGRAPHY 147 BIBLIOGRAPHY Anderson, K., Lytton, H., 8 Romney, D. (1986). Mothers interaction with normal and conduct-disordered boys: Who affects whom?. Developmental Psychology, 22, 604-609. Badien, N. A. (1988). A prediction of good and poor reading before kindergarten entry: A nine-year follow-up. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 21, 98-103. Bagozzi, R. P., 8 Yi, Y. (1988). On the evaluation of structural equation models. 5 Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 16, 74-94. H 4‘ Baker, P. C., 8 Mott, F. L. (1989). NLSY Child Handbook 1989: A Guide and Resource for the National Longitudinal Survey of Youth 1986 Child Data. Columbus, OH: Center for Human Resources Research, The Ohio State University. 1'. Barbarin, O. A. (1993). Coping and resilience: Exploring the inner lives of African American children. Journal of Black Psychology, 19, 478-492. Belsky, J. (1984). The determinants of parenting: A process model. Child Development, 55, 83-96. Belsky, J. (1990). Parental and nonparental care and children’s socioemotional development: A decade in review. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 52, 885-903. Belsky, J. (1997). Determinants and consequences of parenting: Illustrative findings and basic principles. In W. Hellinckx, M. J. Colton, 8 M. Williams (Eds), lntemational perspectives on family support (pp. 5-23). Brookfield, Vermont: Arena. Bloome, D. (1989). Classrooms and literacy. Nonlvood, NJ: Ablex. Bolger, K. E., Patterson, C. J. Thompson, W. W., 8 Kupersmidt, J. B. (1995). Psychosocial adjustment among children experiencing persistent and intermittent family economic hardship. Child Development. 66(4), 1 107(23). Bowen, R., South, M., Fischer, 0., 8 Looman, T. (1994). Depression, mastery and number of group sessions attented predict outcome of patients with panic and agoraphobia in a behavioral/medication program. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 39, 283-288. 148 Brooks-Gunn, J. (1995). Children in families in communities: Risk and intervention in the Bronfenbrenner tradition. In P. Moen, G. H. Elder, 8 K. Luscher (Eds), Examining lives in context: Perspectives on the ecology of human development (pp. 467-519). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Brooks-Gunn, J. 8 Duncan, G. J. (1997). The effects of poverty on children. The future of Children, 7(2), 56-71. Brunk, M., 8 Henggeler, S. (1984). Child influences on adult controls: An experimental investigation. Developmental Psychology, 20, 1074-1081. A Caughy, M. O., DiPietro, J. A., 8 Strobino, D. M. (1994). Day-care participation as a protective factor in the cognitive development of low-income children. 17' Child Development, 65, 457-471. I: Chase-Lansdale, P. L., Brooks-Gunn, J., 8 Paikoff, R. L. (1992). Research and programs for adolescent mothers: Missing links and future promises. American Behavioral Scientist, 35, 290-312. Chase-Lansdale, P. L., Gordon, R. A., Brooks-Gunn, J., 8 Klebanov, P. K. (1997). Neighborhood and family influences on the intellectual and behavioral competence of preschool and early school-age children. In J. Brooks-Gunn, G. J. Duncan, 8 J. L. Aber (Eds), Neighborhood poverty: Context and consequences for children (pp. 79-118). New York: Russell Sage Foundation. Conger, R. D., McCary, J. A., Yang, R. K., Lahey, B. B., 8 Kropp, J. P. (1984). Perception of child, child-rearing values, and emotional distress as mediating links between environmental stressors and observed maternal behavior. Child Development, 55, 2234-2247. Cotterell, J. L. (1986). Work and community influences on the quality of child rearing. Child Development, 57, 362-374 Cox, M. J., Owen, M. T., Lewis, J. M., 8 Henderson, V. (1989). Marriage, adult adjustment, and early parenting. Child Development, 60, 1015-1024. Crnic, K., 8 Acevedo, M. (1995). Everyday stresses and parenting. In M. H. Bomstein (Ed.), Handbook of parenting: Volume 4 (pp. 277-297). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. Crockenberg, S. (1987). Predictors and correlates of anger toward and punitive control of toddlers by adolescent mothers. Child Development, 58, 964- 975. 149 Crockenberg, S., 8 McCluskey, K. (1986). Change in maternal behavior during the baby’s first year of life. Child Development, 57, 746-753. Dodge, K. A., Pettit, G. S., 8 Bates, J. E. (1994). Socialization mediators of the relation between socioeconomic status and child conduct problems. Child Development, 65, 649-665. Dubow, E. F ., 8 lppolito, M. F. (1994). Effects of poverty and quality of the home environment on changes in the academic and behavioral adjustment of elementary school-age children. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 23, 401-412. Duncan, G. J., Brooks-Gunn, J., 8 Klebanov, P. K. (1994). Economic deprivation and early-childhood development. Child Development. 65(2), 296-318. Dunn. L., 8 Markwardt, F. (1970). Peabody individual achievement tests. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service. Fleming, A. S., Gordon, L. F., Ruble, D. N.. 8 Shaul, D. L. (1988). Postpartum adjustment in first-time mothers: Relations between mood, maternal attitudes, and mother-infant interactions. Developmental Psychology, 24, 71-81. Garmezy, N. (1983). Stressors of childhood. In N. Garmezy 8 M. Rutter (Eds), Stress, coping, and development in children (pp. 43-83). NY: McGraw-Hill. Garmezy, N. (1991 ). Resiliency and vulnerability to adverse developmental outcomes associated with poverty. American Behavioral Scientist, 34(4), 416-430. Garrett, P., Ng’andu, N., 8 Ferron, J. (1994). Poverty experinces of young children and the quality of their home environments. Child Development, 65. 331-345. Goodman, S. H., 8 Brogan, D., Lynch, M. E., 8 Fielding, B. (1993). Social and emotional competence in children of depressed mothers. Child Development, 64, 516-531. Hamish, J. 0., Dodge, K..A., Valente, E., 8 Conduct Problems Prevention Research Group. (1995). Mother-child interaction quality as a partial mediator of the roles of maternal depressive symptomatology and socioeconomic status in the development of child behavior problems. Child Development, 66, 739-753. Heath, S. B. (1989). Oral and literate traditions among Black Americans living in poverty. American Psychologist, 44, 367-373. 150 Hoyle, R. H. (1995). Structural equation modeling: Concepts, issues, and applications. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications, Inc. Jackson, A. P., Gyamfi, J. P., Brooks-Gunn, J., 8 Blake, M. (1998). Employment status, psychological well-being, social support, and physical discipline practices of single black mothers. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 60(4), 894-902. Joreskog, K. G., 8 Sorbom, D. (1988). LISREL Vll: A guide to the program and applications. Chicago:SPSS, Inc. Kaiser, A. P., 8 Delaney, E. M. (1996). The effects of poverty on parenting young children. Peabody Joumal of Education, 71(4), 66-85. Lester, B. M., McGrath, M. M., Garcia-Coll, C., Brem, F. S., Sullivan, M. C., 8 Mattis, S. G. (1995). Relationship between risk and protective factors, developmental outcome, and the home environment at four years of age in term and preterm infants. In H. E. Fitzgerald, B. M. Lester, 8 B. Zuckerman (Eds), Children of poverty: Research, health, and policy issues (pp. 197-231). New York: Garland Publishing, Inc. Luster, T., Bates, L., Fitzgerald, H., Vandenbelt, M., 8 Key, J. P. (2000). Factors related to successful outcomes among preschool children born to low- income adolescent mothers. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 62, 133- 146. Luster, T., 8 Mittelstaedt, M. (1993). Adolescent mother. In T. Luster 8 L. Okagaki (Eds), Parenting: An ecological perspective, (pp. 69-100). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Luster, T., 8 McAdoo, H. (1996). Family and child influences on educational attainment: A secondary study analysis of the High/Scope Perry Preschool data. Developmental Psychology, 32(1), 26-39. Masten, A. S., 8 Coatsworth, J. D. (1998). The development of competence in favorable and unfavorable environments: Lessons from research on successful children. American Psychologist, 53(2), 205-220. McLeod, J. D., 8 Shanahan, M. J. (1993). Poverty, parenting and children’s mental health. American Sociological Review. 58(3), 351-366. McLoyd, V. C. (1990). The impact of economic hardship on black families and children: Psychological distress, parenting, and socioeconomic development. Child Development, 61, 311-346. 151 McLoyd, V. C. (1998). Socioeconomic disadvantage and child development. American Psychologist, 53(2), 185-204. Menaghan, E. G., 8 Parcel, T. L. (1991). Stability and change in children home environment: The effects of parental occupational experiences and family conditions. Paper presented at the Biennial Meeting of the society for Research in Child Development, Seattle, Washington. Patterson, G. R., DeBaryshe, B. D., 8 Ramsey, E. (1989). A developmental perspective on antisocial behavior. American Psychologist, 44, 329-335. Peterson, J. L., 8 Zill, N. (1986). Marital disruption, parent-child relationships, and behavior problems in children. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 48, 1- 295-307. It. Pettit, G. S., Bates, J. E.. 8 Dodge, K. A. (1997). Supportive parenting, ecological context, and children’s adjustment: A seven-year longitudinal study. Child Development, 68(5), 908-923. Purcell-Gates, V. (1995). Other people’s words: The cycles of low literacy. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Purcell-Gates, V., 8 Dahl, K. L. (1991). Low-SES children’s success and failure at early literacy learning in skills-based classrooms. Journal of Reading Behavior, 23, 1-34. Radloff, L. (1977). The CES-D scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Joumal of Applied Psychological Measurement, 1, 385-401. Rubin, K. H., Stewart, S. L., 8 Chen, X. (1995). Parents of aggressive and withdrawn children. In M. H. Bomstein (Ed), Handbook of parenting: Volume 1 (pp. 255-284). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. Safron, D. J. (1999). Youth work and psychological well-being. Unpublished Doctoral Dissertation. Michigan State University. East Lansing, MI. Sampson, R. J., 8 Laub, J. H. (1994). Urban poverty and the family context of delinquency: A new look at structure and process in a classic study. Child Development, 65, 523-540. Sulzby, E., Branz, C. M., 8 Buhle, R. (1993). Repeated readings of literature and low socioeconomic status of Black kindergarteners and first graders. Reading and Writing Quarteriy: Overcoming Leaming Difficulties, 9, 183- 196. 152 Takeuchi, D. T, VVlIliams, D. R., 8 Adair, R. K. (1991). Economic stress in the family and children’s emotional and behavioral problems. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 53, 1031 -1041. Teale, W. H. (1986). Emergent literacy: Reading and writing development in early childhood, National Reading Conference Yearbook, 36, 45-74. Vondra, J., 8 Belsky, J. (1993). Developmental origins of parenting: Personality and relationship factors, In T. Luster 8 L. Okagaki (Eds. ), Parenting: An ecological perspective, (pp. 1-34). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Walford-Kraemer, P., 8 Light, H. (1984). Depression and mastery in women: Differences according to personal characteristics. Psychological Reports, 54, 710. Webster-Stratton, C. (1990). Stress: A potential disrupter of parent perceptions and family interactions. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 19, 302-312. Werner, E. E., 8 Smith, R. S. (1992). Overcoming the odds: High risk children from birth to adulthood. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press. Yoshikawa, H. (1994). Prevention as cumulative protection: Effects of early family support and education on chronic delinquency and its risks. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 28-54. Zigler, E.. Taussig, C., 8 Black, K. (1992). A promising preventive for juvenile delinquency. American Psychologist, 47, 997-1006. 153 111111'jjjjljjjjjll‘jjjljll