‘ . fit» “an an , H... . «t. IV." 3 .PA. a ‘5 (u a. 3...; 1. u... 45‘ a. #4933? . I in. 33!. .1 32.30018: 1 l . , . . I. , I : 1% u ‘ 14??) § (9. winnrwdrdfl; ic’fiiatfx‘ll p.19.) .. .\§ iii-:0... bit: ‘sp'lnvl.’«um; (‘1 v.) . its} . ,Efifiafifii )1 .t: «Hrgmfifiwk. qov’v This is to certify that the dissertation entitled Changes in lateral bias associated with changes in task difficultyfor the perception of ch1mer1c faces: Which cognitive processes are respon81b1e? presented by Timothy J. Carbary has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for Ph.D. degree in Psychology ééiuflmraidZL4a7£¢~NaJ a'or,professor ui Lauren 1us Harris Date ?/8/Z‘N L MSU is an Affirmative Action/Equal Opportunity Institution 0-12771 LIBRARY Michigan State University PLACE IN RETURN BOX to remove this checkout from your record. To AVOID FINES return on or before date due. MAY BE RECALLED with earlier due date if requested. DATE DUE DATE DUE DATE DUE 6/01 cJCIRClDateDuopes-p. 15 CHANGES IN LATERAL BIAS ASSOCIATED WITH CHANGES IN TASK DIFFICULTY FOR THE PERCEPTION OF CHIMERIC FACES: WHICH COGNITIVE PROCESSES ARE RESPONSIBLE? by Timothy James Carbary A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Psychology 2002 ABSTRACT CHANGES IN LATERAL BIAS ASSOCIATED WITH CHANGES IN TASK DIFFICULTY FOR THE PERCEPTION OF CHIMERIC FACES: WHICH COGNITIVE PROCESSES ARE RESPONSIBLE? By Timothy J. Carbary In the flee-viewing chimeric faces test (CFT), adults make judgments about mirror-image chimeras of human faces. On this test, adults usually rely more on cues in their left visual hemispace so that their judgments show a left visual hemispace, or LVH, bias. The bias has been explained as a product of a change in the balance of arousal or activation between the cerebral hemispheres leading to an attentional shift to the side of space contralateral to the more aroused hemisphere. The LVH bias thus is seen as a product of the greater arousal of the right hemisphere. Like lateral biases in other perceptual tasks, the CFT bias is known to vary in strength and even direction, for individual stimuli as well as for some individual subjects. Inspection of variations of lateral biases in studies that use other stimuli and other stimuhis presentation methods suggests that the variations in CFT biases might be related to task difficulty. This Task Difi‘iculty Hmothesis was tested in 7 experiments using methods for assessing the existence of the effect as well as its strength and direction. Three possrble outcomes were considered: that task difficulty is associated with increased right hemisphere arousal, resulting in a stronger LVH bias, on the premise that difficulty invokes the discrimination of relevant from irrelevant information; that it is associated with increased left hemisphere arousal, resulting in a weaker LVH bias, on the premise that task difliculty invokes the use of either a feature-search or verbal analysis; or that it is associated with decreased right hemisphere arousal on the premise that the task becomes more diflicult as the quality, or “facedness,” of the stimulus is diminished. Two kinds of chimeric face tests were used. In a 3-face test, subjects judged the similarity of each pair of chimeric faces to an unaltered target face. In a 2-face test, they chose the better exemplar of emotion, femininity, or age in pairs of chimeric faces. Experiments 1- 4 defined parameters for the remaining experiments and showed that task difficulty does affect the strength of the LVH bias, although the direction of the effect began to be clear only in Experiment 4, which showed that difficulty was associated with a weaker LVH bias. Experiment 5 confirmed the effect and its direction, as did Experiments 6 and 7, which showed that the effect also generalizes across different stimuli and methods. Overall results thus supported the Task Difliculty Hypothesis and showed that task difficulty is associated with a weaker LVH bias. With the effect and direction established, the experiments then considered the question of mechanisms. To this end, subjects in Experiments 6 and 7 were asked which of several predefined cognitive strategies they thought they had used during the test; their accounts were consistent with the model, that is, with the hypothesized changes in hemispheric arousal associated with an LVH bias weakened by task difficulty. The subjects’ own accounts I suggest that the change reflects the adoption of a “first-impression” strategy for easy judgments and a feature-search and/or verbal strategy for difficult judgments. Further analysis also suggests a contribution of “facedness” to the effect. Clinical implications of the results are discussed along with suggestions for further research to validate the underlying assumptions about cortical activity related to the behavioral findings. DEDICATION This work is dedicated to my parents, Jim and Nancy Carbary, and to my fiancee, Faye Berry. Each, in their own way, has generously supported my eflons. iv ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS Many people have helped me in my research and in other ways throughout my graduate career. My Dissertation Committee Chair, Professor Lauren Julius Harris, has shown unwavering dedication to his students, holding them to the same high standards exemplified in his own work. He has supported my work and that of his other students with matchless enthusiasm and skill. In particular, through the many drafts of this dissertation, he taught me that to write clearly one must think clearly, and that it is a life- long endeavor. My friend and fellow graduate student, Jason Almerigi, has contributed to this work at all levels, and I deeply appreciate his fiiendship and support. Over the last several years, I also have had the pleasure of working with several outstanding undergraduate students. For the experiments comprising my dissertation, I am especially indebted to Nick Erber and Lori Buhlman. Audrey McNabb, Nathan Besonan, and Todd Sanders also contributed in significant ways in our laboratory, and their help was very important. I also am very grateful to the members of my Dissertation Committee, Professors Norman Abeles, Tom Carr, and Joel Nigg, for their insight, recommendations, and guidance in the design of the final experiments. Without the cooperation of the students who served as subjects in the experiments, the research could not have been done. These include hundreds of undergraduates at Michigan State University who participated through the Department of Psychology subject pool, as well as dozens of high school students from Alpena High School, Alpena, Michigan. The assistance fiom Melissa Doubek, Psychology Instructor at Alpena High School, is much appreciated. I am particularly glad for the opportunity to have helped her integrate parts of the research into her own classroom lectures. I also want to thank Professors Linda Gerard and Andrew Barclay for inviting their students to participate in the research and for giving me the opportunity to lecture on the research to their classes. For all the students, I hope they found their participation valuable. Without face models — the many fellow students, faculty, stafl', and friends who lent their mugs for use in this research - the work itself could not have been done. Here’s looking at you! The seven experiments described in this dissertation have been reported in a different form at the poster session of the ninth (1998), eleventh (2000), and twelfih (2001) annual meetings of TENNET, a conference of experimental and theoretical neuropsychology, Montreal, Quebec. Accounts also have been published in special issues of Brain and Cognition (Experiments 1 and 2: Carbary, Almerigi, & Harris, 1999; Experiments 4 and 5: Carbary, Almerigi, & Harris, 2001; Experiments 6 and 7: Carbary, Almerigi, & Harris, in press). I am grateful to the anonymous referees for TENNET and Brain and Cognition for their helpfiil comments and suggestions. For Experiments 6 and 7, I also thank Academic Press and Professor Harry A. Whitaker, Editor of Brain and Cognition, for permission to alter and use the cartoon faces designed by Ley and Bryden (1979) for their research published in the same journal. Finally, I am pleased to acknowledge the contribution of Dr. David Write and his fellow members of the University Committee for Research Involving Human Subjects (UCRIHS), the internal review board for the use of human subjects at Michigan State University. All of the research was conducted with their approval and oversight, and their reviews of the experimental procedures were invariably timely and helpful. TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES ............................................................................................................. ix LIST OF FIGURES ............................................................................................................ x I. INTRODUCTION .......................................................................................................... 1 II. WHY ARE THERE LATERALITY EFFECTS? ......................................................... 3 Nature of Stimulus Processing ............................................................................... 3 Part- Whole Processing ......................................................................... .3 F eature-Configural Processing ............................................................... 4 Local—Global Processing ....................................................................... 4 High — Low Spatial Frequency Processing ............................................ 5 Task Difficulty ........................................................................................................ 6 Individual Differences ............................................................................................ 9 Strength of Lateralization ........................................................................ 10 Tonic State of Hemispheric Arousal ....................................................... 10 What is the Proximate Processing Mechanism for Visual Hemispace biases? Dynamic Activation-Attention Model ................................................................. 10 III. TESTING THE TASK DIFFICULTY HYPOTHESIS .............................................. 14 Proposed Role of Mechanisms as Applied to Visual-Verbal Stimuli ................... 14 A ttenuation Hwothesis .......................................................................... l4 Normalization Hwothesis ...................................................................... 15 Proposed Role of Mechanisms as Applied to Face Stimuli .................................. 15 Attenuation Hmothesis .......................................................................... 16 Normalization Hypothesis ...................................................................... 17 F eature-Analysis ijothesis .................................................................. 17 Overview of the Current Research ........................................................................ 17 IV. PARAMETRIC STUDIES: EXPERIMENTS 1 — 4 .............................................. 20 Experiment 1 ......................................................................................................... 20 Method ................................................................................................... 21 Results ................................................................................................... 23 Discussion ............................................................................................. 24 Experiment 2 ........................................................................................................ 24 Method .................................................................................................... 24 Results .................................................................................................... 24 Discussion .............................................................................................. 26 Experiment 3 ........................................................................................................ 26 Method .................................................................................................... 26 Results ................................................................................................... 29 Discussion .............................................................................................. 30 Experiment 4 ........................................................................................................ 32 Method .................................................................................................... 32 Results .................................................................................................... 32 Discussion ............................................................................................. 35 vii V. PRINCIPAL STUDIES: EXPERIMENTS 5 — 7 ......................................................... 36 Experiment 5 ........................................................................................................ 38 Method .................................................................................................... 38 Results .................................................................................................... 38 Discussion .............................................................................................. 40 Experiment 6 ........................................................................................................ 41 Method .................................................................................................... 41 Pilot Studies to Establish Task Difliculty ............................................... 42 Results .................................................................................................... 45 Discussion ............................................................................................ .47 Experiment 7 ........................................................................................................ 48 Method ..................................................................................................... 48 Results .................................................................................................... 48 Discussion ............................................................................................. 52 VI. SUMMARY AND GENERAL DISCUSSION ........................................................... 53 Status of Hypotheses ............................................................................................. 53 Normalization ijothesis ........................................................................ 53 F eature-A nalysis Hwothesis ................................................................... 54 Attenuation Hypothesis .......................................................................... 54 Interpretation of Data From Other Studies ......................................................... .55 VII. CONCLUSIONS AND QUESTIONS FOR FUTURE RESEARCH ........................ 58 Difficulty and Cognitive Strategy ......................................................................... 58 Individual Differences .......................................................................................... 58 Attention-Arousal Model ...................................................................................... 60 Accuracy of Self-Report ....................................................................................... 61 Clinical Implications ............................................................................................. 61 APPENDIX A ............................................................................................... 63 REFERENCES .............................................................................................................. 65 viii LIST OF TABLES Table l, p. 18 ................. Predicted Effects of Task Difliculty on Hemispheric Arousal and Visual Hemispace Bias for Visual- Verbal Stimuli and for Faces. Summary of predicted changes in bias for difficult judgments according to each hypothesis: Attenuation and Normalization Hypotheses for verbal stimuli, and Attenuation, Normalization, and F eature-Analysis Hypotheses for faces. Table 2, p. 51 ................ Percentage of Subjects Reporting Use of Each Strategy on Dzflicult Judgments for Experiments 6 and Difficult and Easy Judgments for Experiment 7. Summary of data fi'om self-report questionnaire, which asked subjects what cognitive strategy they thought they used when making difficult judgments in Experiment 6 and when making difficult and easy judgments in Experiment 7. ix LIST OF FIGURES Figure l, p. 22 .................. Example of a 3-face set. Face A, an L-L chimera, was made by merging the left halfof the target (top) face and its mirror reversal along the midline. From the viewer’s perspective, its left side is identical to the left side of the target face. Face B, an R-R chimera, was made fi'om the right side of the target face. From the viewer’s perspective, its right side is identical to the right side of the target face. Figure 2, p. 27 .................... Example of 2-face sets. Example of an HN “happy-neutral” 2-face set is shown on the left. Each chimera was constructed of a smiling half-face and a neutral half-face (of the same face model) joined along the midline. On a single test page an HN chimera and its mirror reversal were shown, one above the other. For one of the HN chimeric faces, the smiling halfappeared in the subject’s lefi visual hemispace (LVH), the other in the right visual hemispace (RVH). Therefore, if subjects chose as the happier chimera the one with the smiling halfto their left, that would constitute an LVH choice. Examples of MP and Y0 face-sets are shown in the middle and to the right. Figure 3, p. 43 .................. Examples of Realistic and Line-Drawn Cartoon F ace-Sets. Cartoon face-sets are shown for three levels of judgment difficulty: easy (top), medium (middle), and difficult (bottom), for realistic cartoon chimeras (left) and for line-drawn chimeras (right). Figure 4, p. 46 .................. Strength of L VH biases for easy through difi‘icult judgments for photographic and cartoon face-sets for Experiment 6. The figure shows the relation between strength of LVH bias and level of difficulty. Non-overlapping standard error bars indicate statistical reliability. Figure 5, p. 50 .................. Strength of L VH biases for easy through difi'icult judgments for photographic and cartoon face-sets for Experiment 7. The figure shows the relation between strength of LVH bias and level of difficulty. Non-overlapping standard error bars indicate statistical reliability. Figure 6, p. 56 ................... Strength of L VH biases for photographic, realistic cartoon, and line-drawn cartoon face-sets for Experiments 6 and 7. The data are collapsed over difliculty level, showing the potential relation between strength of LVH bias and degree of “facedness.” I. INTRODUCTION Of all the principles about the hurmn brain, perhaps the most familiar is that the cerebral hemispheres play different roles in cognitive fimctioning, with the left hemisphere taking the lead for speech and language, the right hemisphere for a variety of non-verbal tasks. This generalization has found support for well over a century in studies of firnctioml deficits in brain-injured patients, since the 19503 in behavioral studies of normal persons and callosotomy patients, and most recently in functional radiological imaging studies of patients as well as normal persons. Thus, when speech and language deficits result fiom brain disease, the associated lesions are typically in the left hemisphere, and when patients show deficits in perception and recognition of faces, objects, and other visual-spatial forms, the lesions are typically in the right hemisphere (see Benson & Zaidel, 1985; Hellige, 1993; Harris, 1999a, for reviews). Likewise, in studies of normal persons, when complex visual images are shown tachistoscopically in the right or left visual fields (RVF, LVF), letters and words are usually processed more quickly and accurately in the RVF (e.g., Hellige & Webster, 1979; Rizzolatti, Umilta, & Berlucchi, 1971; Umilta, Sava, & Salmaso, 1980; Wagner & Harris, 1994; Zurif & Bryden, 1969), whereas non-verbal targets, including faces, arrangements of dots, and orientations of lines, are usually processed more quickly and accurately in the LVF (e.g., Boles, 1994; Rizzolati & Buchtel, 1977) Similarly, for free- viewing tasks using chimeric faces as targets, most persons show left visual hemispace (LVH) biases consistent with the left visual field (LVF) biases formd in tachistoscopic, divided visual field studies (e.g., Carlson & Harris, 1985; Hoptrnan & Levy, 1988; Jaynes, 1976; Luh, Redl, & Levy, 1994; Luh, Rueckert, & Levy, 1991). Finally, in flyfRI experiments, verbal tasks have been associated with greater left- than right—hemisphere activation (Puce, McCarthy, Allison, & Gore., 1996; Breier et al., 1999), whereas non-verbal tasks, including nonverbal tasks including several of those listed earlier, have been associated with greater right than left hemisphere activation (Haxby, Horwitz, Ungerleider et al., 1994; McCarthy, Puce, Gore, & Allison, 1997; Nakamura, Kawashima, Ito, et al, 1999; Puce et al., 1996). Similar effects have been reported in PET studies (Haxby, Grady, Horwitz, et al., 1997; Sergent, Ohta, & McDonald, 1992) and in studies using surface electrode recordings (e.g., Bentin, Allison, Puce, & McCarthy, 1996). 1]. WHY ARE THERE LATERALITY EFFECTS? Practically as soon as the facts of lateral specialization were established, hypotheses were proposed to account for the phenomenon, that is, to explain why the hemispheres are differently specialized for language and spatial functions. For at least the last two decades, it has been acknowledged that explanations that focus on the verbal/spatial classification are inadequate primarily because not all lateralized functions have obvious verbal or spatial content. Two examples are praxis and perception of melody, which are lateralized predominately to the left and right hemispheres, respectively. Nature of Stimulus Processing The focus of most of the new hypotheses has been less on the content of the lateralized function than on how the cerebral hemispheres process the information, on the premise that differences in content will be compatible with different stimulus-processing modes. Over the years, a number of versions of this basic idea have been proposed (for an early review, see Bradshaw and Nettleton, 1981; for a more recent review, see Hellige, 1993). The fan examples that follow are only some of the versions proposed. None of them, ftu‘thermore, are intended to dichotomize the brain but, rather, to suggest a continuum of function between the hemispheres. Their focus, like the focus of this dissertation, also is on perceptual, especially visual, ftmctions rather than on output, or motor fimctions. Part- Whole Processing On the premise that all visual stimuli require some degree of decomposition into their constituent parts for effective processing, Farah and colleagues (Farah, 1994; Farah, Levinson, & Klein, 1995; Farah, Tanaka, & Drain, 1995; Farah, Wilson, Drain, & Tanaka, 1998) have proposed that the poles of the dimension of specialization are “part- analysis” and “whole-analysis” represented by left and right hemisphere processes, respectively. Using printed texts and faces as examples, the hypothesis is that texts, which require extensive decomposition (into letters, syllables, words, phrases) for analysis, are normally processed more effectively by the left hemisphere, whereas faces, whose parts must be analyzed together, are normally processed more effectively by the right hemisphere. It follows that a face is recognized as a face or as a face expressing a certain emotion not so much fiom any individual part but from all the parts seen together. For certain emotional expressions, some parts also may be more important than others or may play a special role (e.g., contraction of the obicularis occuli muscles, which is present in a spontaneous, or Duchenne, smile, but absent in a so-called “false” smile). Feature-Confrgural Processing Gauthier and colleagues (Gauthier, Behrrnan, & Tarr, 1999; Gauthier & Tarr, 1997; Gauthier, Williams, Tarr and Tanaka, 1998) have described the essential processing difference between the hemispheres not in terms of parts and wholes but in terms of stimulus features and stimulus configurations. Although not committed to seeing these processes as lateralized, they do not object to this characterization (personal communication with M. Tarr, September 2, 1999). Local - Global Processing Robertson, Lamb, and Knight (1988) have proposed that the dimension is best characterized as local-global. They based this proposal on their finding that for letter- stirnuli composed of smaller letters, patients with left-hemisphere damage had difficulty perceiving the smaller letters that form the larger letter (“local” perception) but not the larger letter itself (“global” perception). Conversely, patients with right-hemisphere damage had difficulty perceiving the larger letter but not the smaller letters. From this, they concluded that perception of the global configuration (larger letter made of smaller letters) is deficient in right-hemisphere patients whereas perception of local features (small letters that form the larger letter) is deficient in left-hemisphere patients. High - Low Spatial Frequency Processing Save for variations in the names assigned to the poles of the dimension, the three models described above characterize the difference between the hemispheres in terms of the analysis of parts and the integration of those parts into a whole. Others have proposed that the more fundamental difference lies at the level of sensory information independent of any such “part” — “whole” classification. One such model, by Sergent (1991) as well as Robertson and Delis (1986), focuses on the spatial frequency of the stimulus and proposes that higher frequency information is better processed by the left hemisphere and lower frequency information by the right. In a further development, Irvy and Robertson (1998) have proposed a two-stage model according to which an initial analysis is made in terms of spatial fiequency, followed by a second spatial fiequency analysis dictated by the demands of the perceptual task (e.g., deploying attention to constituent parts, or to their configuration, as is indicated to be required after the first analysis). They call this the “double filtering by frequency” model. By their nature, the parts of a whole, or a configuration, are physically smaller than the whole itselfand therefore subtend a smaller visual angle. They therefore occur more fi'equently and require a higher frequency analysis than the configuration of those parts; the configuration, by definition, occurs Iessfrequently and requires a relatively lower frequency analysis. That said, the three previously described models and the spatial frequency model are not incompatible; the spatial frequency model may come closer to describing how stimulus characteristics are better suited for analysis of parts versus the configuration of those parts. Task Dificulty If the strength and even the direction of the laterality effect are influenced by the kinds of processing called for by the task, there is indirect evidence that they also are influenced by the overall difficulty of the task, as indexed by accuracy and reaction time. From a clinical perspective, this is important because task difliculty is progressively scaled in most neuropsychological tasks. Failures on more dificult tasks are less reliable indicators of a disease process than are failures on easier tasks. In clinical practice, it is generally assumed that when tasks are easy, so that most patients perform correctly, they perform the tasks in a similar way. When tasks are more difficult, however, and patients begin to fail, they are more likely to fail for different reasons. For example, failures may reflect sub-standard premorbid achievement (i.e., never having had the requisite level of skill in the first place). They also rmy reflect the patient’s inability to change or shift cognitive strategy to solve difficult tasks in a different way, either because the alternative strategy itself is dysfunctional or because the ability to change strategies is dysfirnctional. The proposal that task dificulty is related to changes in cognitive processing strategies may also help generate explanations for the unusual co-occurrence of low-end errors with high-end success on some psychometric instruments. In such cases, the strategy for difficult items may be the only strategy available to the patient, and after initial low-end errors, then a change is made to process the task differently, making for higher-end successes. The evidence for difficulty effects on laterality tasks comes fi'om several of the research paradigms reviewed in Part I. All the evidence, however, is indirect. For example, for naming tachistoscopically-projected letters, when the letters are made more complex or when their font is degraded, presmnably making the task more difficult, the usual RVF bias has been found to weaken or even change to an LVF bias (Bryden & Allard, 1976; Hellige & Webster, 1979; Wagner & Harris, 1994). Bryden and Allard (1976) suggested that this change reflects a change in processing style and that complex or visually-degraded stimuli must be “normalized” and perceived as an exemplar of a known stimulus category (e.g., letters must first be recognized as letters) before they can be separated into relevant and irrelevant components for the usual left-hemisphere processing. For face perception, on divided visual field (tachistoscopic) tasks, the usual LVF bias has been formd to change when faces are inverted (Leehey, Carey, Diamond & Calm, 1978; Hillis, Hiscock, & Rexer, 1995; Luh, 1997), when subjects are cued to attend to facial features rather than to the whole face (Rhodes, 1985), when the task is to discriminate between schematic faces that differ on a single feature (Patterson and Bradshaw, 197 5; Bradslmw and Sherlock, 1982; F airweather, Brizzolara, Tabossi, & Umilta, 1982), and when subjects are shown two different views of a face and asked whether the faces are the same or different (Bertelson,Vanhaelan, & Morais, 1979). Each such condition presumably could make the task more difficult. In these studies difficulty had been operationalized as lowered task accuracy and/or increased response latency. Given the premises of the stimulus processing hypotheses, the laterality data also suggest that more and less difficult tasks are performed differently. The nature of the difference is less clear. Broadly, across stimulus classes, does a more difficult task require a broad configural analysis to identify a stimulus class, or does it require a detailed feature analysis to do so, or is there an interaction between task content (e.g., visual, motor, or verbal) and processing style? Furthermore, will the answers to these questions be the same for all stimulus categories, or will the dynamics be different for different stimulus classes? Whatever may be the basis for the hypothesized difficulty-related changes in laterality effects, over time and as experience with the stimulus material increases, tasks also normally become less difficult as subjects develop facility or familiarity with them. On this view, it lms been suggested that laterality efl‘ects therefore depend, at least in part, on the subject’s level of expertise (Diamond & Carey, 1986; Gauthier et al., 1998; Gauthier & Tarr, 1997; Bruyer & Crispeels, 1992; Rhodes & McLean, 1990). For visual- spatial material, some investigators have proposed that the hallnmk of expertise is configural processing of the sort for which the right hemisphere appears to be specialized (Gauthier et al., 1999), whereas non-experts focus on stimulus features, a left hemisphere function, without regard for the relationship of the featm‘es to one another. Gautier and colleagues studied the role of expertise in MRI experiments using “nonsense” visual stimuli before and after subjects were allowed to develop expertise in discrimination (Gauthier, Anderson, Tarr, et al., 1997; Gauthier, Tarr, Moylan, et al., 2000). As already noted, laterality effects were not the main focus, but in these studies, right hemisphere activation appeared to take the lead (M. Tarr, personal communication, September 2, 1999). For faces, that is to say human faces, however, it seems safe to say that virtually all normal adults are “experts.” Right hemisphere, configural processing therefore presumably would be modal, and, for that reason, so would LVF or LVH biases on face perception tasks, which is what the literature indicates. Collectively, the tachistoscopic and free-viewing studies suggest a relation between what I have taken task difficulty to be task difficulty and the direction and strength of lateral biases. This suggested relation hereafter will be referred to as the Task Difliculty Hypothesis. What the studies do not clearly show is whether or why the effect should be in one direction or another. In the context of the models of cerebral specialimtion described earlier, one could say, for example, that where increased task difficulty is associated with a decreased LVF or LVH bias, the change is toward local feature analysis, whereas when it is associated with an increased LVF or LVH bias, the change is toward holistic, configural processing. That is to say that task difliculty is associated with a change in processing style and in turn a change in processing style leads to a change in VH bias. These examples obviously do not exhaust the range of possibilities, and the predictions may be different for verbal and non-verbal tasks. Individual Differences The proposed role of expertise raises the more general issue of individual difl'erencesandhowtheymight figure inthe assessment ofthe Task Difficulty Hypothesis. At least two sources, or kinds, of difference can be envisioned. Strength of Lateralization One kind is the strength of lateralization. Laterality effects of the kind described here are generally stronger for right-handers than for left lenders. The difference may reflect the greater heterogeneity in unselected samples of left-handers along with the less strictly lateralized function in these individuals (see review in Harris, 1992). Modest sex differences also have been reported, with females showing weaker laterality effects for verbal material but also stronger effects for the perception of emotion represented in faces (Bmton & Levy, 1989; Natale, Gur, & Gur, 1983). Tonic State of Hemispheric Arousal Another kind of difference is in characteristic hemispheric arousal. Levy and colleagues have presented evidence that individual differences on lateralized tasks reflect differences in tonic state of left versus right hemisphere arousal (Levy, Heller, Banich, & Burton, 1983; Hoptrmn & Levy, 1988). Some persons, therefore, may have a characteristic, or default, hemispheric style, whereas for other persons, the style may be more malleable and therefore more affected by their subjective experience of task difficulty. What is the Proximate Processing Mechanism for Visual Hemispace Biases? The Dynamic Activation-Attention Model I£ according to the Task Difficulty Hypothesis, as has been suggested, task difficulty, moderated by individual differences, affects the strength and even the direction of the lateral bias in visual tasks, what is the proximate mechanism underlying the effect? For the tachistoscopic divided visual field paradigm, the conventional interpretation of lateral biases, in consideration of the anatomy of the visual system, is that they are direct products of structural-anatomical connections and the resultant representation of the visual fields in contralateral visual cortex. Thus, for faces and other non-verbal targets, the usual left visual field (LVF) bias is seen as a product of hemisphere-specific IO information accessing only the preferred (right) hemisphere, accompanied by signal decay prior to any interbemispheric transfer of information. That explanation, however, cannot obviously account for the left visual hemispace (LVH) bias for the flee-viewing of chimeric faces or for the presence of changes in strength and even direction of the bias in both tachistoscopic and fi'ee viewing tasks. For these reasons, a strict structural-anatomic explanation has largely been replaced by more dynamic models. The particular model adopted for the current series of experiments was first proposed by Trevarthan (1972) and elaborated by Kinsbourne (1970, 1973, 1974) to account for laterath efi‘ects on tachistoscopic as well as auditory (dichotic listening) tasks, and then further developed by Levy and colleagues and applied to free-viewing tasks with verbal as well as non-verbal material (Levy et al., 1983; Levy & Kueck, 1986). The model, which may called a dynamic attention-activation model, assumes that the levels of arousal in the cerebral hemispheres are generally in reciprocal balance, leading to a fixation of attention straight-ahead in extra-personal space. It then assmnes that the balance will be disturbed by stimulation fi'om one or the other side of space or by endogenous unilateral arousal caused by the expectation of a particular kind of stimulus. Finally, it assumes that the imbalance leads to a change in orientation and attention contralateral to the more aroused hemisphere and therefore to a bias in readiness to accept and process input from the visual hemispace on that side. (In keeping with the premises of the model, the general term, visual hemispace, or VH, and the directional terms LVH and RVH, will be used to refer to the lateral biases on tachistoscopic divided visual field tasks as well as on free-viewing tasks.) In other words, the hemisphere specialized for processing particular stimuli becomes differentially aroused, or “primed,” when that 11 stimulus class is presented; this arousal is associated with a shift of attention to the opposite side of space. For a flee-judgment task, whether the flees are presented tachistoscopically or for free-viewing, to the extent that the right hemisphere is aroused more than the left, attention will be driven to the LVH, enhancing the salience of “face” information to the viewer’s left side and giving a processing bias to information on that side. 1 Two assumptions thus underlie the use of the dynamic activation model to explain the VH biases for judgments of flees and visual-verbal material. The first is that there is greater activation of one hemisphere in response to the stimulus or task - more right- hemisphere activation for flees, more left-hemisphere activation for verbal material. The second is that this activation causes attention to be directed to the contralateral side of space. Direct support for the first assumption comes fiom data cited earlier from firnctional imaging and EEG studies. Indirect support comes from clinical data, also cited earlier, showing deficits in flee perception and visual-verbal comprehension associated with right and left brain injury, respectively. Direct support for the second assumption is harder to obtain, and it depends on how attention is defined. There is, however, indirect support. For visual tasks, attention is usually associated with eye movements towards the visual target, so that subjects showing an LVH bias would be expected to look more or longer to the part of the object appearing in the left hemispace. As Posner and his colleagues have shown, however, ' Kinsbourne’s model has not gone unchallenged. For a relatively recent test of the dynamic activation- attention model, see the series of 5 experiments and discussion in Reuter-Lorenz, Kinsbourne, & Moscovitch (1990), wherein they demonstrate three principles of the model: I) that attention is biased to 12 attention can be directed without eye movements (Posner, Walker, Freidrich, & Rafal, 1983). In these experiments, however, subjects were trained to keep their eyes still while attending to peripheral stimuli. In a more naturalistic setting, deployment of attention is normally associated with changes in eye movements, as in a typical orienting response. For this reason Kinsbourne (1970) proposed that eye movements might be a reliable index of direction of attentional deployment, so that even though attention and eye movements need not be synchronized, under normal circrunstances in a flee-viewing flee perception test, eye movements would be expected to follow the locus of attention. The Task Difficulty Hypothesis therefore predicts changes in lateral deployment of attention related to the perceived difliculty of a visual-spatial task. 2 the direction contralateral to the stimulated hemisphere, 2) that biases are not task dependent, and 3) that in the caseof a directional conflict, the rightward bias (RVH bias) is stronger. 2 Research by Harris, Pierce, and Henderson (unpublished data, L. J. Han'is personal communication, l999b) suggests that the relation between eye movements and performance on the lice-viewing tasks dissertation is very complex. The majority of their subjects did move their eyes from the vertical midline and did show the usual LVH bias on a face-judgment task, but eye movement and performance were not directly related in any obvious way. 13 III. TESTING THE TASK DIFFICULTY HYPOTHESIS As already noted, variations in stimulus and test parameters have been associated with changes in the strength or even direction of the VB bias for verbal as well as non- verbal material, including flees. The main premis of this dissertation, as expressed in the Task Difficulty Hypothesis, is that the changes are directly related to task difficulty. If, as the hypothesis suggests, task difficulty affects the bias by affecting how the task is performed, that might help to account for the individual differences and stimulus-related variations in the strength of the LVH bias on flee perception tasks. Within the context of a dynamic activation-attention model, however, the hypothesis makes different predictions for verbal and non-verbal material. As will be explained below, the hypothesis cannot differentiate between two possible explanations for VH bias changes for verbal material, and there is a third possibility that can be tested only in a non-verbal task, which is why, for the current work, the hypothesis was tested with flees. Proposed Role of Mechanisms as Applied to Visual-Verbal Stimuli Based on the dynamic activation-attention model, there are at least two ways that task difficulty could weaken the LVH bias for visual-verbal material - by attenuation of the character of the stimulus and by normalization of the stimulus. Attenuation Hypothesis Assume that the verbal stimulus is a sequence of letters. Whether it is made more complex (e.g., by adding ornate strokes and curls) or is degraded (e.g., by lowering the intensityordiflirsingthefont), itisassmnedthattheletterswillbelesslikelyto arouse left-hemisphere “letter processing” areas to the extent that they look less like letters, that is, to the extent that their “letterness” has been attenuated. By default, that would leave 14 the right hemisphere more aroused than the left, causing the perceptual bias to change from the RVH to the LVH. Normalization Hypothesis Alternatively, the right hemisphere might become more active not relatively but absolutely, and not by deflult but directly, if the more complex or degraded stimulus directly activates the right hemisphere instead of the left hemisphere where the stimulus is usually processed. Bryden and Allard (1976) suggested that under these circumstances, what occurs is stimulus normalization, a pre-processing of the stimulus if it is not innnediately recognized as a letter. They described normalization as a separation of relevant from irrelevant parts, or signal from noise, and they assumed that it is primarily a right-hemisphere process. For degraded or unusually complex verbal stimuli, they further assumed that this process is activated to make sense enough of the stimulus to determine whether it is verbal (letter or word) or something else. The resulting right- hemisphere arousal thus manifests as an LVH bias. In surmnary, to the extent that making the visual-verbal task more difficult is associated with a change in perceptual bias fiom RVH toward LVH, the change may be due either to an attenuation of the stimulus character and therefore to lowered arousal of the left hemisphere, or to a normalization of the stimulus category, thereby recruiting a different cognitive process fimctionally represented in the right hemisphere. In either case, the result is a shift of the arousal ratio to the right. Proposed Role of Mechanisms as Applied to Face Stimuli For verbal material, both the Attenuation and Normalization Hypotheses predict the same change in bias for difficult tasks - fiom RVH toward LVH. For non-verbal 15 material, such as flees, they make different predictions. For flees, there also is a third way for task difficulty to affect the bias. Attenuation Hypothesis Just like letter recognition can be made more dificult to recognize by making the letters look less like letters, so flees can be made more dificult to recognize or to discriminate by making them look less like flees, that is by attenuating their “flcedness.” One way to accomplish this is to invert them. Inversion may attenuate flcedness in at least two ways. First, it may hinder processing at the entry level, therefore requiring at least additional time to determine whether the stimulus is or is not a flee. Second, even after an inverted flee is determined to be a flee, inverting it or inverting some of its features may hinder quick and accurate judgments offlcc qualities such as its emotion (Thompson, 1980). To this extent, it’s less flee-like. Some inversion studies have shown that the usual LVH bias weakens or even changes to an RVH bias (Rhodes, Brake, & Allison, 1993), although other studies have not found any change (Kolb, Milner, & Taylor, 1983). Where a change has been found, it Ins been suggested that the inverted flee fails to arouse those right-hemisphere processes nornnlly activated for upright flees (Rossion, Delvene, Debatisse, Gofl‘aux, et aL, 2001). In terms of the dynamic activation model, the assumption is that the reduction in right-hemisphere arousal shifts the balance to the left hemisphere, driving the viewer’s attention to the RVH. If, for flees, attenuation of flcedness proves not to be associated with a change in VB bias, then two hypotheses remain - the Normalization Hypothesis and the Feature- Analysis Hypothesis (i.e., analysis of features that comprise the flcial configuration). 16 Normalization Hypothesis Just as it does for letter processing, making flee diseriminations difficult by degrading them or by making them highly complex through the addition of irrelevant features would require separation of task-relevant from task-irrelevant information. Unlike letter processing, however, if, by this normalization process, right-hemisphere mechanisms are recruited, then the result should be to strengthen, not weaken, the usual LVH bias. F eature-Analysis ijothesis For faces, as noted above, there is a third way for difficulty to affect the bias. Making the task more difficult could precipitate a feature-based analysis. As suggested above, when a face is decomposed and parsed for its features (e.g., nose, mouth), a left hemisphere analysis is most likely to be used. Therefore, in contrast to the Normalization Hypothesis, which predicts that the LVH bias will be strengthened, a F eature-Analysis Hypothesis predicts that it will be weakened. In summary, each of the hypothesized mechanisms — Attenuation and Normalization for verbal stimuli, and Attention, Normalization, and Feature-Analysis for faces — makes a prediction about changes in the VH bias when perceptual tasks are made more difficult. These predictions are summarized in Table 1. Overview of the Current Research The current research consists of a series of seven experimental tests of the Task Difficulty Hypothesis for flee-viewing chimeric flee tasks. Experiments 1- 4 were designed to test the hypothesis and at the same time to establish stimulus and measurement parameters. With the parameters established, Experiments 5 — 7 were 17 Table 1. Predicted Effects of Task Difficulty on Hemispheric Arousal and Visual Hemispace Bias for Visual-Verbal Stimuli and for F aces Visual-Verbal Stimuli Predicted Effects hmthesis hemispheric aroufl visual hemis ace bias Attenuation J, L — R T LVH l RVH Normalization — L T R T LVH I RVH Face Stimuli Predicted Efl'ects hypothesis hemispheric ampfl visual hemis ace ias Attenuation - L l R 1 LVH T RVH Normalization 1 L T R T LVH I RVH Feature-Analysis T L J, R I LVH T RVH L and R are left and right hemispheres; LVH and RVH are left and right visual hemispace biases; the dash (—) represents no hypothesized clunge, and arrows represent predicted increases or decreases in hemispheric arousal and visual hemispace bias. l8 designed to test the hypothesis more precisely. Experiments 6 and 7, however, incorporated three important changes. First, instead on relying on the subjects’ own judgments of task difliculty, difficulty levels were established by another group, independently of the experimental subjects. Second, in order to achieve experimental control over the level of difficulty for the judgments, specially designed cartoon flees were used in addition to photographic flees. Finally, to identify the cognitive strategies used by the subjects, subjects were questioned about the strategies they used while making their judgments for the tasks they found to be difficult and easy. All seven experiments also were designed to assess different possible explanations of the Task Difliculty Effect by 1) ruling out the Normalization Hypothesis or by 2) ruling out the Attenuation and Feature-Analysis Hypotheses. For flees, it was supposed that a weakened LVH bias in association with increased task dificulty will falsify the Normalization Hypothesis, leaving the Attenuation and the F eature-Analysis Hypotheses, whereas it was supposed that a strengthened LVH bias will fllsify the Attenuation and the F eature-Analysis Hypotheses, leaving the Normalization Hypothesis. Given an outcome where the Attenuation and Feature-Analysis explanations remain, further studies would be needed to test their relative contributions. 19 IV. PARAMETRIC STUDIES: EXPERIMENTS l — 4 Before we can address the question which, if any, of the hypothesized explanations underlies the task difficulty efl‘ect, we must establish that the effect exists in the first place and determine whether it is associated with a stronger LVH bias, consistent with the Normalization Hypothesis, or a weaker LVH bias, consistent with the Attenuation and F eature—Analysis Hypotheses. To do this, stimulus, subject response, and efl‘ect-size parameters must be defined. As just noted, this was the purpose of Experiments 1 — 4. These experiments used two versions of a flee-viewing chimeric flce test that has been found to yield a reliable LVH bias. Experiments 1 and 2 used 3-flce sets, and, based on these results, Experiments 3 and 4 directly compared 3-face and 2- face sets. Based on these results, Experiments 5 -— 7, comprising the principal part of this dissertation, used 2-flee sets exclusively. The results of Experiments 1 — 3 also led to changes in the measurement of task difliculty. All three experiments used an indirect measure oftask difficulty, whereas Experiment 4 — 7 used a direct measure, as will be explained below. Experiment 1 In Experiment 1, with 3-face sets, subjects were asked to compare two chimeric flees to an unaltered target flee. The target face was located at the top of the stimulus display with the chimeric faces below it. One chimeric flee, an “L-L chimera,” was made from the left side of the target flee and the mirror image of that side; the other, an “R-R chimera,” was made from the right side of the target flee and the mirror irmge of that side. After a set of L-L and RR chimeras was made, the flees were cropped to a uniform size and oval shape, primarily to exclude extra-flee cues including the outline of 20 the model’s hair. (See Kowner, 1995, for a discussion.) Chimeras were rmde fiom black and white photographs of real flees, digitally scanned and manipulated in AdobeO Photoshop. Face models were volunteers from the academic community, including faculty, staff, and students. An example of a 3-flce set is shown in Figure 1. For each flee-set, subjects performed a two-stage task, first judging how similar the L-L and R-R chimeras were to each other, and then judging which chimera looked more like the target flee. The assumption was that the more similar the chimeras were to each other, the more difficult they would be to discriminate fi'om each other or item the target flee, thereby making the task more difficult. The subjects’ own ratings of the similarity of the L-L and R—R chimeras thus were used as an indirect measure of task difficulty. The effect of task difficulty on the VH bias, therefore, was assessed by examining the relation between the subjects’ L-L and R-R similarity ratings and the strength and direction of their VH bias. An LVH bias meant that the L-L chimera was reliably chosen over clmnce. Strength of the bias was measured by the overall number of L-L choices. Method Subjecm. Subjects were 80 undergraduates (72 right-handers, 8 left handers, 46 females, 34 males). Procedure. Subjects were tested in a classroom in groups of five to ten. Five 3- face sets were used. Each flee-set was projected onto the classroom screen via overhead projector. Subjects first were shown a sample flee-set with the chimeric flees visible but with the target flee covered. They were asked to rate how different the chimeras were from each other on a 7-point scale. After 10 seconds, the target face was exposed and the 21 Figure 1. Example of a 3-face set. Face A, an L-L chimera, was made by merging the left half of the target (top) flee and its mirror reversal along the midline. From the viewer’s perspective, its left side is identical to the left side of the target flee. Face B, an R-R chimera, was made fiom the right side of the target flee. From the viewer’s perspective, its right side is identical to the right side of the target flee. 22 subjects were asked which chimeric face looked more like the target flee. After this, testing continued in the same manner with the five 3-face sets. Results For each of the five 3-flce sets, a one sample t-test was performed to compare the proportion of subjects showing a VH bias in either direction to the proportion expected by chance (.50). Two of the flee-sets showed a reliable LVH bias, two showed a reliable RVH bias, and one showed no bias in either direction. There was no detectable relation between task difficulty based on the L-L and RR similarity ratings and strength of VH bias, and there were no reliable effects of sex or handedness. Afterwards, it became clear that a different test could be used to assess the relation between the chirneras’ difference-ratings and the strength of VH bias across face-sets. To that end, flee-sets were rank-ordered in two ways: by mean strength of LVH bias and by mean rated differences between the L-L and R-R chimeras. Rankings were such that a positive correlation would mean that an increasing LVH bias was associated with a decreasing difference between the chimeras and/or that an increasing RVH bias was associated with an increasing difference between the chimeras. Although the Pearson r correlation between the two variables was an encouraging +.69, it was not reliable and had a reasonable probability (p = .2) of being a random efl‘ect. Discussion Although reliable VH biases were found, they were not consistemly in the same direction across flee-sets, and no relation was found between the VH biases and the similarity ratings. In contrast, however, to the planned correlational analysis where the N 23 was 80, the post-hoe analysis yielded a promising but statistically unreliable relation, but because the N was only 5, the statistical power of the analysis was greatly reduced. Experiment 2 For Experiment 2, instead of a post-hoe analysis like the one in Experiment 1, a planned comparison was made. To determine the number of flee-sets necessary to achieve acceptable statistical power for the analysis, a power calculation was performed based on the results of Experiment 1. The results indicated that twenty flee-sets would give a power of 75% to detect a correlation of .50 in the predicted direction. Method Subjects. Subjects were 59 undergraduates (53 right-handers, 9 left handers, 46 females, 34 males). Procedure. Subjects were tested in a classroom in groups of five to ten. The procedure was identical to Experiment 1 but with twenty flee-sets instead of five. Result Of the twenty flee-sets, six showed a reliable LVH bias, six a reliable RVH bias, and eight showed no bias in either direction. Again, there were no reliable effects of sex or handedness. Of the twelve flee-sets showing a reliable LVH or RVH bias, for only two was the bias strength related to task difficulty, one a stronger LVH bias, the other a stronger R VH bias. All twenty flee-sets were rank-ordered by mean strength of LVH bias and by mean rated differences between the chimeras. The Pearson r correlation between the two variables was .01 (p<.98), again non-reliable. The same correlational analysis then was 24 performed for only those twelve flee-sets that elicited a reliable LVH or RVH bias. The result was a non-reliable Pearson r of .16 (p<.62). Discussion Although Experiment 1 showed a high but unreliablecorrelation fiom an unplanned post-hoe test, consistent in direction with the Normalization Hypothesis, the results of Experiment 2, with better statistical power, were non-reliable, with no obvious directional trend. Both experiments, however, also yielded overall weaker LVH biases than obtained previously in our laboratory when face-sets were presented in booklet form rather than via overhead projection, as in Experiments 1 and 2, and this may have contrrhuted to the absence of a reliable relation between task difficulty and VB bias.3 If so, then establishing a reliable LVH bias must be the first requirement for testing the Task Dificulty Hypothesis. To help meet this requirement in the subsequent experiments, all test stimuli therefore were presented in booklet form. Along with this change, flee-sets were scrutinized for whether or not they produced a reliable VH bias in Experiment 2. Twelve did, and one nearly did. This left seven 3-flce sets that, for unknown reasons, showed little propensity to elicit a bias. Those seven were culled, leaving thirteen flee-sets for continued use. Other studies in our laboratory also have found larger overall VH biases with 2-flce than with 3—face sets.4 If the overall VH bias is more reliable with one format than with another, the more reliable format should boost statistical power due to reduced dispersion of the data. However, if a more reliable LVH 3 In Experiments 1 and 2, strength of VH bias for flee perception also may have been influenced by what Boles (1994) described as input variables (e.g., stimulus characteristics like luminescence and size), possibly by the large size of the projected flee-set or by the contrast of a dim testing room with an illuminated flee-set. ‘ There may be several reasons for the difference in the strength, reliability, and direction of the VH bias between the 2-flce and 3-flce sets. That discussion, however, is beyond the scope of this paper. 25 bias is confirmed with 2-flce sets, a different measure of task difficulty also would have to be developed. These considerations guided the design of Experiment 3. Experiment 3 In Experiment 3, along with the thirteen 3-face sets from Experiment 2, fourteen 2-flce sets were used, consisting of eight “happy-neutral” (HN) flee-sets, three same-sex young-old (YO) flee-sets, and three male-female (MF) flee-sets. The HN chimeras were made from a smiling half-face merged along the vertical midline with a neutral half-face. Both half-flees were of the same flce model, whereas the Y0 and MF flee-sets were necessarily made from different flee models. All 2-flce sets consisted of a chimera and its mirror reversal, one above the other. For the HN flee-sets, subjects were asked which flee looked happier; for the YO flee-sets, which flee looked younger; and for the MF flee-sets, which flee looked more feminine. Examples of each kind appear in Figure 2. For each kind, choosing the chimera with the relevant eue(s) (happier half, younger half, more feminine half) to the subject’s left would constitute an LVH choice. The 2-flce sets were used so that the strength, direction and reliability of LVH bias could be compared for the 3-flce and 2-flce formats. As already noted, if one format is more reliable, its use should boost statistical power due to reduced dispersion of the data. Method Subjects. Subjects were 103 undergraduates (96 right-handers, 7 left-handers; 88 females, 15 males). Procedure. Two test booklets were prepared to counterbalance the left — right position of chimera for the 3-face sets and the top — bottom position of chimeras for the 26 Figure 2. Examples of 2-face sets: happy-neutral (HN) on the left, masculine-feminine (ME) in the middle, young-old (YO) on the right. All chimeras were constructed in the same way except that for the HN chimeras the two halves came from the same face model. Thus, each HN chimera consisted of a smiling half-flee and a neutral half-flee of the same flee model joined along the midline. On a single test page an HN chimera and its mirror reversal were shown, one above the other. For one of the chimeras, the smiling halfappeared in the subject’s left visual hemispace (LVH), the other in the right visual hemispace (RVH). Therefore, if subjects chose Face A as the happier chimera, that is, the one with the smiling halfto their left, that would constitute an LVH choice. Similarly, for the MF and Y0 chimeras, choice of flees B and A as, respectively, the more feminine and older flees would constitute LVH choices. 27 2-flce sets on the stimulus page. In each booklet, the thirteen 3-flce sets came first, followed by the eight 2-flee sets, for a total of 21 sets. Similarity ratings (to be used as the index of task difliculty) were only rmde for the 3—flce sets because the chimeras in the 2-flce sets are mirror reversals and cannot reasonably be judged for similarity. The difliculty measure used for 3-flce sets therefore is not compatible for use with a 2-flce set. All subjects were tested together in a large classroom. As in Experiments 1 and 2, the subjects were told that they would be asked to perform a two-stage task. Beginning with a sample 3-flce set, they were asked to indicate on a 7-point scale how similar the two flees (L-L and R-R chimeras) were to each other, and then to choose which flee was more similar to the third, target flee. In contrast to Experiments 1 and 2, to help subjects understand the scoring method and to help reduce variability by standardizing responses, subjects were shown a flee-set of identical L—L and R-R chimeras and were told that they were examples of two identical flees that should be scored 1, or “identical,” on their response sheet. Then they were shown very dissimilar L-L and RR chimeras and were told they should be scored 7, “low similarity?” After that, an entire 3-flce set was shown.‘5 The L-L and R-R chimeric flees were pointed out at the bottom of the flee-set, and the subjects were asked to rate their similarity on a scale fi'om l to 7 and to record their answer on their response sheets by circling one of the points on a numbered line. After they completed the thirteen 3-flce judgments, subjects made judgments for the eight 2-flce HN chimeras, three YO chimeras, and three MF chimeras for a total of 5 The chimeras used for the “identical” demonstration were identical. The chimeras used for the “low similarity” demonstration were the L-L and R-R chimeras from Experiment 1 rated least similar to one another. Both faces used for these demonstrations were then retired from the bank of test flees. 28 fourteen 2-flce judgments. As with the 3-flce judgments, subjects first were shown via overhead projection a sample 2-flee set for HN chimeras. The flees were labeled A and B and were placed one above the other. Subjects were told that some of the test items would look like this example and that, depending on the flee-set, they would be asked to judge which flee was happier, which flee was older, or which flee was more feminine. (See again the examples in Figure 2.) Then, for a single practice trial, they were asked to judge which flee was happier and to indicate their choice by circling A or B on their response sheet. Subjects then were guided. through their stimulus booklet, item by item, with spoken instructions fi'om the test administrator. They were instructed to turn the pages together and to wait for instructions before doing so. For example, for the 3-flce sets, the test administrator said, “T urn the page. Item six, how different are the two flees at the bottom of the page? (pause) OK, which flee at the bottom of the page looks most like the one at the top?” Subjects were allowed ten seconds to mark their response. Results 3-Face Sets. Of the thirteen 3—flce sets, six showed a reliable LVH bias, three showed a reliable RVH bias, and four showed no bias in either direction. This analysis had a probability of .77 to detect an effect of .15 in the predicted direction. Again, there were no reliable effects of subject sex or handedness. For each of the thirteen 3-flee sets, Pearson rs were calculated to assess the relation between the difference-rating for the chimeras and the strength of the VB bias for that flee-set. Only one correlation was reliable (r = - .25, p<.05), for one of the three 3- flce sets that yielded an RVH bias. In other words, for none of the thirteen flee-sets was 6 Overhead projection was used only for the sample items only, so that the entire group received the same instructional examples. 29 the strength of the difference-rating between the L-L and R-R chimeras directly related to the strength of the LVH bias, but for one flee-set, a greater difference was related to a stronger R VH bias. This analysis, with an n of 103, had an 87% probability to detect a correlation of .3 in either direction. Correlatiorfll analysis yielded the same results for men and women when their data were analyzed separately. There was, however, a slight sex difference for rated similarity of LL and R-R chimeras (on a 7-point scale, men’s mean = 4.52, women’s mean = 4.05, t = 2.09, 101 df, p<.04). As planned, all thirteen 3-flee sets were rank-ordered by mean strength of LVH biases and also by the mean difference-rating between their chimeras. The Pearson r correlation between the two variables was -.39 (p = .18), again non-reliable. This test, however, lfld power of only 56% to detect a correlation of .50 in the predicted direction. The same correlational analysis then was performed for only those nine 3-face sets that elicited a reliable LVH or RVH bias. The result was a non-reliable Pearson r of -.54 (p = .13). Finally, for these same nine 3-flce sets, the mean chimera difference-ratings were compared for the six LVH-sets and the three RVH-sets. The comparison fliled to show any difference (t = 1.9, 7de p = .10). This analysis, however, with seven degrees of freedom, had very low statistical power. 2-Face-Sets. Unlike the 3-flce sets, all fourteen 2-flce sets (HN, YO, MF) showed overall strong, reliable LVH biases. Of the eight HN flee-sets, subjects made LVH choices 65% of the time (t = 5.3, 102de p<.001), with the difference reliable for six face-sets and borderline for the remaining two (p = .06 and p = .09). Of the three YO 30 flee-sets, subjects made LVH choices 74% of the time, with the bias reliable for all three flee-sets (t = 7.8, 102df, p<.001). Of the three MF face-sets, subjects made LVH choices 70% of the time , again reliable for all three flee-sets (t = 6.8, 102df; p<.001). Discussion As expected, the booklet administration yielded VH biases for the majority (nine of thirteen) of 3-flce sets, with the majority being LVH. Again, however, the results did not support the Normalization Hypothesis. Instead, as in Experiments 1 and 2, of the 3- face sets that showed a VH bias, difficulty was related more often to a weaker rather than a stronger LVH bias, the reverse of what is predicted by the Normalization Hypothesis but consistent with the Attenuation and F eature-Analysis Hypotheses (see Table 1). As expected, the overall LVH bias also was stronger for 2-flce than for 3-flce sets: twelve of the fourteen 2-flce sets but only 6 of the 13 flee-sets showed reliable LVH biases, and the individual biases for the 2-flee sets were stronger. Based on these findings, it was decided to test the Task Difficulty Hypothesis with the 2-face as well as the 3-flce task. As already noted, this would require a new measure of task difficulty, because the measurement developed for the 3-flce task (assessment of similarity of LL and R-R chimeras) would not be possible for the 2-flce task. In Experiment 4, therefore, a more direct measure, nflgnitude estimation, was used for both the 3- and 2-flee sets. Subjects performed two tasks for each flee-set (3-face and 2-flee sets): first, they made the appropriate flee judgment; then, using magnitude estimation, they rated the difficulty of that judgment. Use of this dimculty measure allowed for data analysis similar to that used in Experiments 1 -— 3 but now for both kinds of flee-sets. 31 Experiment 4 Method Subjects. Subjects were 121 high school students in introductory and advanced psychology courses (115 right-handers, 6 left-handers; 78 females, 43 males).7 Procedure. Subjects were tested in much the same way as in Experiment 3. However, after seeing a sanrple 3—flce set and judging which chimera looked more like the target flee, subjects were asked how they would rate the difficulty of their judgment (A or B) if the average difficulty were given a rating of twenty points, and to record that number on their response sheet. After the 3-flce task, subjects were shown a sample 2-flce set for HN chimeras. After choosing the happier flee, they were given the parameters noted above and asked to rate the difficulty of that judgment. For the next fourteen 2-flce sets, subjects were allowed ten seconds per judgment, asked to rate the dimculty, then prompted to turn to the next flee-set in unison. All subjects were tested in their classrooms, and their teacher, after training in our laboratory, administered the test. Results 3-Face Sets. For each of the thirteen 3-flce sets, a one sample t-test was used to compare the proportion of subjects showing a VH bias in either direction to the proportion expected by chance (.50). Five flee-sets showed a reliable LVH bias, three showed a reliable RVH bias, and five showed no bias in either direction. This test had 73% power to detect a difference of. 15 in either direction. No sex or handedness 7 At the time Experiment 4 was planned, the oppommity presented itself to test high school students. There was no a priori reason to suppose that the results would difl‘er because of the approximately 3-year age difference between them and the undergraduates used in the other experiments and in light of evidence that hemispace bias does not change within this age range (Levine & Levy, 1986). 32 differences were found; however, the test for a handedness effect was weak due to the limited number of left-handers. For each of the 3-flee sets, Pearson rs were calculated to assess the relation between the subjects’ estimated difficulty (now measured by magnitude estimation) of each judgment and the strength of the VH bias for each flee-set. Of the thirteen correlations, three were reliable (p<.05), two of them, however, for face-sets that did not elicit a reliable left or right VH bias. In other words, for only one of the thirteen flee-sets was difficulty related to a weaker LVH (r = -.21, p< .02). This test had statistical power of 92% to detect a correlation of .30 in either direction. In Experiment 4, unlike Experiment 3, there was no difference between the men’s and women’s overall estimations of task difficulty. As planned, all thirteen flee—sets were rank-ordered by mean strength of LVH bias and by the mean rated differences between their chimeras. The Pearson r correlation between the two variables was -.34 (p = .25), again non-reliable. However, this test had statistical power of only 44% to detect a correlation of .50 in either direction. The same correlational analysis on only those eight flee-sets that elicited a reliable LVH or RVH bias now yielded a non-reliable Pearson r of -.36 (p = .38). Finally, for these same eight flee-sets, the mean chimera difference-ratings were compared for the five LVH-sets and the three RVH-sets. The comparison fliled to show any difference (t = 1.1, 6df, p = .31). This analysis, with 6 degrees offi'eedom, had very low statistical power. 2-Face Sets. Overall, the fourteen 2-face sets (HN, Y0, and MF) again showed strong and reliable LVH biases. For the 8 HN flee-sets, subjects chose the chimera with 33 the smile in their LVH 67% of the time (t = 6.9, 120df, p<.001), with seven of the eight HN face-sets eliciting reliable LVH biases. The 3 YO face-sets elicited an LVH bias 79% of the time (t = 11.2, 120df, p<.001). The 3 MP face-sets elicited an LVH bias 75% of the time (t = 10.4, 120d£ p<.001). As in Experiment 3, every Y0 and MF face-set elicited an LVH bias. Only for the MF face-sets, however, were there sex differences in VB bias strength, with women showing a stronger LVH bias for the MF 2-face sets (men’s mean = 71% LVH choices, women’s mean = 84% LVH choices, t = 2.55, 118df, p<.012). Unlike Experiments 1 — 3, the subjects rated the fourteen 2-face sets as well as the 3-face sets for perceived task difliculty. For each 2-face set (HN, Y0, and MF), Pearson rs were calculated to assess the relation between the subjects’ estimated dimculty of each judgment and the strength of the VH bias for that judgment. The relationship was reliable for only one face-set (r = -.23, p<.013), indicating that for that face-set, the more dimcult the judgment, the weaker the LVH bias. This analysis had statistical power of 92% to detect an effect size of r = .30 in either direction. The pattern of correlations between VH bias and task difficulty was the same for men as it was for the entire group. No face-set, however, showed such a relationship when women’s data were analyzed separately. When all fourteen 2-face sets were rank-ordered by mean strength of LVH bias and mean estimated difficulty, the Pearson r correlation between the two variables was -.32 (p = .27), again non-reliable. This test had power of only 47% to detect a correlation of .50 in either direction. 34 Discussion As in Experiments 1 - 3, the results failed to support the Normalization Hypothesis for either 2- or 3-face sets and instead showed a consistent but infrequently reliable relationship in the direction predicted by the Feature-Analysis and Attenuation Hypotheses. For the 3-face sets, one correlation was consistent with those hypotheses. The results also showed that the magnitude estimation method was successful. For the 2- face sets, the relation between task difliculty and VH bias strength was found to be in the same direction as for the majority of 3-face sets in Experiment 3. Again, only one of the correlations, now for the 2-face sets, was reliable and again consistent with the F eature- Analysis and Attenuation Hypotheses. 35 V. PRINCIPAL STUDIES: EXPERIMENTS 5 - 7 So far, none of the experiments support the Normalization Hypothesis. That is, they fail to show that increased task difficulty is associated with a strengthened LVH bias. Before either the Feature-Analysis or Attenuation Hypotheses can be accepted as alternatives, however, we must be confident that the Normalization Hypothesis can be definitively rejected. To find out will require further experiments. These experiments can be designed with stimuli, parameters, and estirmted effect-sizes based on findings fi'om Experiments 1 — 4, but, as noted in the Overview of Current Research in Part 111, they incorporated four important changes based on the following considerations. First, is the relation between LVH bias and task difficulty peculiar to these methods of assessing VH bias and task difficulty, or does it generalize to other methods? For example, could the use of self-ratings for task difficulty play a role because subjects perform the task expecting some items to be easier and some to be more difficult? To find out, difficulty ratings would have to be standardized prior to the experiment, and subjects would have to be kept naive to variation in task dificulty. Second, there are at least two possible explanations of the relation between task dificulty and a weaker LVH bias that involve increased left-hemisphere activity. One is that the weaker LVH bias reflects increasing left hemisphere arousal because of the introduction of a feature-based analysis; another is that it reflects the introduction of a verbal analysis. In the context of the dynamic attention-activation model, the activation of left-hemisphere language strategies may accormt for the same data as the Attenuation and Feature-Analysis Hypotheses, which means that a left-hemisphere, language mediated strategy will have to be considered as well. To better understand the nature of 36 the processing used and to find out whether one of the remaining strategies was, in fact, being used, subjects in Experiments 6 — 7 were asked directly to describe the strategy, or style of processing, used for their judgments, Third, in Experiment 5, as in Experiments 1 - 4, the chimeric faces were made from photographs of real faces. For those faces, the quality of the displayed emotion varied in unknown and uncontrolled ways. In Experiments 6 —- 7, to achieve a greater measure of control over the displayed emotion, two different kinds of specially designed drawings of faces were used in addition to the photographic chimeras. Finally, in Experiments 5 — 7, only the HN 2-face sets were used. The reason was that in Experiment 4, HN face-sets yielded LVH biases around 70%, compared to 75% for MF and 79% for YO biases. Use of the HN face-sets therefore would allow for a clearer expression of the effect of difficulty on the LVH bias by lessening the possibility of a ceiling effect. In Experiment 4, HN face-sets, unlike MP face-sets, also did not elicit sex differences. Using only the HN chimeras in Experiment 5 —7 therefore made it more likely that the men’s and women’s scores could be pooled for greater statistical power. Although this would come at the expense of potentially finding a sex difi‘erence, it was decided that this was outweighed by the advantage of pooling the scores. In sum, Experiment 5 was designed to replicate the consensus finding of Experiments 1 - 4 and therefore to confidently rule out the Normlization Hypothesis; Experiments 6 and 7 were designed to test whether the task difficulty efl‘ect generalized beyond the established experimental protocol; and Experiment 7 was designed to better understand the nature of the processing strategies used by the subjects. 37 Experiment 5 Method Subjects. Subjects were 358 university students (281 right-handers, 57 lefi- handers, 13 arnbidexters; 256 females, 95 males). Procedure. Thirty-four HN 2-face sets were prepared in the manner previously described. Each face-set was used only once. Two test booklets were prepared in order to counterbalance the position of face-sets on the stimulus page. (See the example of HN face-sets in Figure 2. See also Appendix A for examples of photographic HN face-sets cormnon to Experiments 5 -7.) Subjects were tested in groups of five to ten in a classroom. They were shown a sample 2-face set and then were told that for all test items, they would be asked to judge which face was happier. After making the judgment for the sample, they were asked how they would rate the difficulty of their judgment if the average difficulty were given a rating of 20 points, and to record that number on their response sheet. Resulm L VH Bias Efl'ects. Over all thirty-four face-sets, the mean LVH bias was 68% (t = 12.1, 351df, p<.001). There was no sex difference in the size ofthe bias (women 68.3%, men 67.8%; t = .14, 349df, p<.89), but there was a handedness effect (t = 2.50, 336df, p<.01). Lefi-handers made LVH choices 60% of the time (different from chance t = 2.37, 56df , p<.02); right-handers made LVH choices 70% of the time (different fiom chance, t = 2.50, 336df, p<.01). Diflicully Ratings. On the difficulty ratings, there was no handedness difference (right-handers’ mean = 21, lefl-handers’ mean = 22, t = .34, 336df, p<.79), but there was 38 a sex difference, with men rating the task as more difficult than women (men’s mean = 24, women’s mean = 21, t = 2.8, 349df, p<.006). ' L VH Bias Efl'ects by Level ofDifficulty. The overall correlation between subjects’ mean difficulty ratings for the thirty-four face-sets and mean LVH bias scores for those face-sets was r = -.12 (p<.02). This analysis had statistical power of 81% to detect an effect size of r = .15 in either direction. For each of the thirty-four face-sets, Pearson rs were calculated to assess the relation between the subjects’ estimated difliculty for each of the thirty-four judgments and the strength of the VH bias for each face-set. Thirteen of the thirty-four correlations were reliable (p<.05), and six more approached significance (p<.10). These nineteen correlation coeflicients were all negative and ranged fi'om -.O9 to -. 19, indicating that the more difficult the judgment, the weaker the LVH bias, or, said differently, the greater the change toward an RVH bias. Data for the right-handed subjects yielded the same results as for the entire group (i.e., thirteen of thirty-four correlations were reliable). When the same analysis was conducted on only the lefi-handers’ scores, only two of the thirty-four correlations were reliable. Finally, when the men’s and women’s scores were analyzed separately (collapsed over handedness), three of the thirty-four correlations were reliable for men, nine of the thirty-four correlations for women. As planned, all thirty-four 2-face sets were rank-ordered according to their mean strength of LVH bias and their mean estimated difliculty. The Pearson r correlation between the two variables was -.22 (p = .21) but was non-reliable. This test had a one- tailed power of only 54% to detect a correlation of .30. When analyzed separately, lett- handers showed a non-reliable correlation (r = -.14, p<.30), and right-handers showed a 39 reliable correlation (r = -.13, p<.03). When analyzed separately neither men nor women showed reliable correlations (men: r = -.20 , p< .27; women: r = -.24 , p< .17 ). Discussion Again, no support was found for the Normalization Hypothesis for 2-face sets; instead the relation between task difliculty and VH bias was in the reverse direction. For about halfthe stimulus face-sets (across all subjects), there were reliable relationships between subject-perceived task difficulty and a weakening of the LVH bias usually associated with this type of chimeric face—set. Furthermore, across all face-sets, only right-handers showed this relationship. In sum, the results showed a small but reliable relation between self-rated task difficulty and a weakening of the LVH bias. There may be several reasons for the presence of a reliable effect in Experiment 5 and its absence in Experiments 1 and 2 and the mixed results in Experiments 3 and 4. Most likely it was due to the relatively low power in the earlier experiments to detect a small effect, and, secondly, to the use of 3-face sets in Experiments 1 -— 3 with an indirect measure of task difficulty. In retrospect, the (non-reliable) post hoc positive correlation (r = + .69) between task difficulty and strength of LVH bias in Experiment 1 was misleading. Even with their shortcomings, Experiments 2 — 4 frequently showed a negative relation between LVH bias and task difliculty. Indeed, the reliable results of Experiment 5 are in the same direction as the majority of statistically reliable results in the earlier experiments. So far, task difficulty has not been independently manipulated; it was based on the judgments of the same subjects who had made the face judgments. In the last two experiments, therefore, task difficulty was manipulated and assessed independently, prior 40 to administration of the protocol. As already noted, it was hypothesized that the change in the LVH bias reflected the incorporation of a feature-based analysis, but another possibility must be considered — that more difficult items engender a strategy incorporating verbal analysis of the stimuli. To better understand the nature of the processing strategies used, subjects were asked about the style of processing they employed while making their judgments. Finally, in Experiments 6 and 7 two different kinds of specially designed face cartoons were used in addition to the chimeras made from photographs of real faces in an effort to achieve a greater measure of control over the displayed emotion, and, therefore, task difficulty. Experiment 6 Method Subjects. Subjects were 212 tmiversity undergraduates (212 right-handers; 158 females, 54 males). Test Faces. Test stimuli were twenty-tln'ee pairs of chimeric faces. Two different kinds of faces were used to make the chimeras. Eight of the twenty-three face- sets were made fiom photographs of real faces. (See again the example in Figure 2.) They were drawn fiom a pool of thirty-four face-sets, each of which had been independently rated for difficulty to judge by the 358 subjects fi'om Experiment 5. The eight chosen for Experiment 6 were the four rated “easiest” and the four rated “most diflicult” to judge (shown in Appendix A). The remaining fifteen face-sets were made fiom five different cartoon faces. Three were from a set of realistic faces originally designed by Ley and Bryden (1979) to depict different levels of emotion ranging from 4l extremely negative to extremely positive. For the current study, only the three faces depicting the neutral, mildly positive, and extremely positive expressions were used. The remaining two cartoon faces were more schematic than the Ley and Bryden faces. They were designed, however, to depict the same three levels of emotion depicted in the Ley and Bryden faces, that is, neutral, mildly positive, and extremely positive. For each of the total of five cartoon faces, three face-sets were made such that the hemi—srnile increased in magnitude incrementally, creating what was intended to be three levels of judgment difiiculty — easy, medium, and difficult. The three pairings for one of the Ley and Bryden faces and for one of the line-drawn cartoon faces are shown in Figure 3; all cartoon face-sets are shown in Appendix A. Pilot Studies to Establish Task Difficulty. For the cartoon faces, two pilot studies were conducted to confirm the three levels of difficulty created by the design of the face-sets. In Pilot Study 1, eighteen subjects were shown three pairs of chimeric faces made from one of the Ley and Bryden faces and were asked to choose the happier face of each pair and then to rank order the three sets according to the dimculty of the judgments. The results were in almost perfect agreement (Pearson r = .98, p<.001) with the intended difficulty ranking. In Pilot Study 2, eighteen other subjects were asked to choose the happier face of each pair of all fifteen cartoon face-sets and immediately afler ‘ each judgment to rate each judgment as “easy,” “medium,” or “difficult.” The face-sets were presented one after the other, in pseudo-random order. These ratings yielded only two levels of difliculty instead of three as in Pilot Study 1. In Pilot Study 2, the “easy” and “medium” face-sets received comparable ratings (F = 7.08, l4df, p<.009). Each, however, was rated as easier to judge than the “difficult” face-sets (LSD post-hoe 42 Figure 3. Examples of Realistic and Line-Drawn Cartoon Chimera Face-Sets. Cartoon face-sets are shown for three levels of judgment difficulty: easy (top), medium (middle), and difficult (bottom), for realistic cartoon chimeras (left) and line-drawn chimeras (right). 43 difference between “easy” and “diflicult”, p<.003; “medium” and “difficult”, p<.028; “easy” and “medium”, p<.254). The results, nonetheless, were in close agreement (Pearson r = .72, p<.001) with the intended order of difficulty. Procedure. All subjects were tested together in a large classroom. They first were shown a sample face-set (labeled face A and face B) via overhead projection, asked to judge which face was happier, and then told that they would be asked to make the same kind of judgment for other pairs of faces depicted in their test booklets. The booklets contained the twenty-three face-sets, each on a separate page. The subjects then were guided through the twenty-three face-sets, one at a time, by the test administrator. They were allowed ten seconds per face-set and were asked to mark their choice (face A or face B) on their response sheet. Following the twenty-three judgments, the subjects were directed to another part of their response sheet and were asked three self-report questions about their more difficult judgments. They were instructed as follows: “ For the judgments you just made, you probably found that some were quite easy to make and some were more difficult. For the judgments that were more diflicult to make, did you: I) talk to yourself about how to decide; 2) study small features of the face while trying to decide; or, 3) decide in some other way?” Subjects were told that “talking to themselves” did not necessarily mean talking aloud and that any sub-vocal, or interml, verbalization should be counted. They also were asked to mark all answers that applied and to briefly elaborate if they marked “decided in some other way.” Subsequently, the three self-report categories will be referred to as “feature-search,” “self-talk,” and “some other way,” respectively. Results L VH Bias Efl‘ects. For all twenty-three faces-sets, the mean LVH bias was 0.66 (t = 11.73, 211df; p<.001). Biases for the eight photographic and fifteen cartoon face- sets were nearly identical, 0.67 for the photographic face-sets, and 0.65 for the cartoon face-sets. Both means were different fiom chance (t = 9.55, 211df, p<.001 and t = 11.13, 211df, p<.001, respectively) but not fi'om each other (t = 1.54, 211df, p = .13). L VHBias Efl'ects by Level of Difficulty. For the photographic face-sets, the LVH bias was 0.71 for the four “easy” sets (t = 10.10, 211df, p<.001) and 0.64 for the ‘ four “difficult” sets (t = 7.04, 211df, p<.001), with both means different fi'om chance. A one-way AN OVA indicated that the biases were reliably different fi'om each other (difference = .064; F = 4.90, 423df; p = .027). For the cartoon face-sets, the LVH bias was 0.67 for the five “easy” sets (t = 8.95, 211df; p<.001), 0.68 for the five “medium” sets (t = 10.16, 211df, p<.001), and 0.60 for the five “difficult” sets (t = 6.35, 211df, p<.001), with all three means different from chance. An AN OVA showed that these biases were different fiom each other (F = 6.27, 634df; p = .002). LSD post-hoe tests also revealed reliable differences between the biases for easy and diflicult face-sets (difference = .07; p = .009) and between the medium and difficult face-sets (difference = .08; p = .001) but not between the easy and medium face-sets (difference = .01, p = .459). The bias strengths are shown in Figure 4. A 2-tailed Pearson correlation for the photographic face-sets showed an inverse relation of r = -.11 between the two levels of task difficulty and LVH bias (n = 424; p = .027). Likewise, for the cartoon face-set for which the pilot studies indicated only two levels of difficulty, the correlation was r = -.14 (n = 636; p<.001). 45 0.75 — g 0.70 4 '6 .r: 0 g 0.65 - .l I.- o 5 0.60 . t o o. 2 0- 0.55 , 0.50 '- ”mt easy difficul easy medium difficul photographic fame» cartoon face-sets Figure 4. Strength of LVH bias for easy through diflicult judgments for photographic and cartoon face-sets for Experiment 6. The figure shows the relation between strength of LVH bias and level of difficulty. Non-overlapping standard error bars indicate statistical reliability. 46 Self-Report Questions. For the more difficult judgments, 47% of the subjects checked “self-talk,” 90% checked “feature-search,” and 23% checked “some other way.” Several subjects who checked “some other way” and explained that they had made their choice based simply on their first impression. See Table 2 (page 52). Discussion In sum, when task difficulty was defined and validated independently, it had the same effect on the VH bias as it did in Experiment 4 and earlier experiments. The results thus further support the Task Difficulty Hypothesis and allow the confident rejection of the Normalization Hypothesis. Based on the self-report data, the results also suggest that the change in the VH bias fiom left to right is more closely related to the use of a feature-search style of analysis than to the use of verbal mediation. The self-report data, however, were incomplete because subjects were asked only about the judgments they found to be diflicult, not the ones they found to be easy. Before the more difficult judgments can be confidently associated with any particular strategy or strategies, self-report data are needed for both kinds of judgments, easy as well as diflicult. The results also showed that subjects who checked “some other way” often noted that their choice was based on their first impression. If “first-impression” had been included among the categories, it therefore might have increased the validity of individual choices. With these considerations in mind, Experiment 7 was designed with two procedural changes. Self-report questions were asked for the easy as well as the diflicult judgments, and “first impression” was added as a 4til category. 47 Experiment 7 Method Subjects. Subjects were 82 university undergraduates (82 right-handed; 66 females, 16 males). Test Faces. Test booklets and face-sets were the same as those used in Experiment 6. Procedure. The testing procedure also was the same except that, as already noted, in the self-report period, subjects were asked about their easy as well as their diflicult judgments and were given “first impression” as a 4th category. Results LVHBias Efl'ects. For all twenty-three face-sets, the overall LVH bias was 0.67, virtually identical to the score in Experiment 6 and, again, reliably different from chance (t = 7.83, 79df; p<.001). Likewise, the biases for the eight photographic and filteen cartoon chimeras were very similar to those in Experiment 6 and, again, very similar to each other, 0.70 for the photographic chimeras and 0.65 for the cartoon chimeras, with both biases again different fiom chance (t = 6.59, 79df, p<.001 and t = 7.12, 82df, p<.001, respectively) but not fiom each other (t = 1.30, 79df, p = .20). L VH Bias Efl'ects by Level of Difficulty. For the photographic face-sets, the LVH bias was 0.71 for the four “easy” face-sets (p<.001; t = 6.01, 79df) and 0.70 for the four “difficult” face-sets (t = 7.04, 79df; p<.001) (For these and some remaining analyses, the degrees of freedom are discrepant due to casewise deletions for missing data points.) This time, a one—way AN OVA indicated that the biases for the easy and dificult face-sets were not reliably different fiom each other (difference = .017; F = 1.50, 158df; p = .223), 48 although the direction of the efl‘ect was the same as in Experiment 6. For the cartoon face-sets, the LVH bias was 0.71 for the five “easy” face-sets (t = 6.859, 79df, p<.001), 0.67 for the five “medium” face-sets (t = 5.372, 79df, p<.001), and 0.58 for the five “dificult” face-sets (t = 3.497, 79df; p = .001). A one-way ANOVA showed reliable difl‘erences between the LVH biases per difficulty level (F = 5.05, 239df; p = .005). LSD post-hoe tests revealed reliable differences between the easy and difficult face-sets (difference = .129; p = .001) as well as between the medium and difficult face- sets (difierence = .087; p = .029). As in Experiment 1, an LSD post-hoe test showed no difference between the biases of the easy and medium difficulty groups (difference = .040; p = .291). These results are shown in Figure 5. For the photographic face-sets, a one-tailed Pearson correlation showed a non- reliable relation, r = -.028, between task difficulty and LVH bias (n = 160; p = .364). For the cartoon face-sets, a one-tailed Pearson correlation for all three levels of difficulty showed a reliable relation between task difficulty levels and LVH bias (n = 240) of r = - .21 (p = .001). Self-Report Questions. For the judgments that the subjects said were more difficult, 51% checked “self-talk,” 92% checked “feature-search,” 17% checked “some other way,” and 66% checked “first impression.” For easier judgments, 22% checked “self-talk,” 28% checked “feature-search,” 2% checked “some other way,” and 93% checked “first impression.” Table 2 lists the percentage of subjects reporting use of each strategy. 49 0.75 . § 0.70 - ‘_ '6 .C o .L. I 0.65 —~ 3 “5 .g 0.60 — E o 8 E 0.55 ‘1 easy difficult easy medum difficdt ”'0me facgsets cartoon face-sets Figure 5. Strength of LVH bias for easy through diflicult judgments for photographic and cartoon face-sets for Experiment 7. The figure shows the relation between strength of LVH bias and level of difficulty. Non-overlapping standard error bars indicate statistical reliability. 50 Table 2. Percentage of Subjects Reporting Use of Each Strategy on Difficult Judgments for Experiment 6 and on Difficult and Easy Judgments for Experiment 7 Experiment 6 Experiment 7 n = 212 n = 82 self-talk strategy diflicult 470/, 51 °/. easy 22% feature search strategy diflicult 90% 92% easy 28% some other way difficult 23% 17% easy 02% first impression diflicult 66% easy 93% 51 Discussion The main results further confirm the task difliculty effect in Experiment 6. They also confirm the finding indicating the predominance of the “feature-search” over the “self-talk” strategy for difficult tasks and show that both strategies, but especially “feature-search,” are associated more closely with difficult than with easy judgments. Finally, they show that for easy judgments, “first impression” was the overwhelming strategy reported. In combination, the results suggest that when the judgment is suficiently easy, subjects can rely only on first impression, precluding the need for any other strategy. It has been long established that feature-oriented processing is associated with the left hemisphere and that configuration-oriented processing is associated with the right hemisphere. This series of experiments shows tlmt task difficulty is associated with chsanges from one hemisphere and processing style to another. Still, however, a very interesting question remains, namely, which leads which? Does task dificulty precipitate a change in hemisphere and style of processing, or does a change in hemisphere and style of processing precipitate the experience of difficulty? The current experiments were not designed to address this question, but addressing it is a very interesting, logical next step. 52 VI. SUMMARY AND GENERAL DISCUSSION Overall, the results support the Task Difficulty Hypothesis, that is, that task difliculty is related to the strength of the VH bias for the judgment of emotion in chimeric faces. After testing the hypothesis while at the same time establishing stimulus and measurement parameters in Experiments 1 - 3, a task difliculty efi‘ect emerged in a consistent direction by Experiment 4, and in Experiments 5 - 7 its strength and direction were established and replicated, with the effect each time being a weakening ofthe LVH bias as task difliculty increased. In Experiment 5, the same subjects made the lace judgments who assessed task difliculty, and in Experiments 6 and 7, difficulty was independently assessed and subjects also were asked to identify the cognitive strategies they thought they had used to perform the task for diflicult judgments in Experiments 6 and 7 and for difficult and easy judgments in Experiment 7. Status of Hypotheses With the task difliculty effect established for different kinds of face-sets and different ways of assessing difficulty, the question is how do the results fit the difl‘erent hypothesized explanations? They clearly rule out the Normalization Hypothesis, lean strongly toward the Feature-Analysis Hypothesis, and possibly suggest a role for the Attenuation Hypothesis. Normalization Hypothesis By the Normalization Hypothesis, more diflicult face-sets, like the more difficult letters and fonts tested by Bryden and Allard (1976) and by Wagner and Harris (1994), would have to be perceptually “normalized” so that relevant and irrelevant information could be distinguished and so that a stimulus class or category could be established to 53 assist firrther processing. The current experiments, however, repeatedly showed that the LVH bias, instead of being strengthened, as predicted by the Normalization Hypothesis, was weakened. F eature-Analysis Hypothesis The direction of the effect thus supports the Feature-Analysis Hypothesis, consistent also with the subjects’ self-reports, in Experiments 6 and 7, of their predominant use of a feature-search strategy and, secondarily, of linguistic cueing for the dimcult judgments. Therefore, if a weakening of the LVH bias reflects an increase in left-hemisphere arousal, these results suggest that the change in bias mostly reflects the introduction of a feature-search strategy. Attenuation Hypothesis Although the results show that task difliculty is related to the use of feature search, the Attenuation Hypothesis has not been ruled out. That is, a weakening of the LVH bias with increasing task difficulty might still be accounted for by a reduction of right hemisphere arousal resulting fi'om diminished “facedness” of the target stimulus. Although a direct test of this possibility was not planned in the designing of the current experiments, a rough kind of test is possible. Recall that in Experiments 1 — 5, the chimeric faces were made from photographs of real faces, whereas in Experiments 6 and 7, in addition to the photographic fiaces, two kinds of specially designed cartoon faces were used: the more realistic Ley and Bryden cartoons and the more schematic line- drawn cartoons. After designing the cartoon chimeras, it became clear that the photographic chimeras looked more like real faces than either kind of cartoon face, with the line-drawn chimeras showing the least resemblance. This permitted comparison of 54 the overall LVH bias (collapsed over task difficulty) across the three levels of “facedness.” The results, summarized in Figure 6, suggest that the LVH bias was stronger for photographic face-sets and realistic cartoon face-sets than for line-drawn cartoon face- sets. For the combined data fiom Experiments 6 (n = 224) and Experiment 7 (n = 89), the overall LVH bias was 0.67 for photographic face-sets, 0.66 for more realistic cartoon face-sets, and 0.62 for line-drawn face-sets. Results fiom a one-way ANOVA indicated that the biases were reliably different fi'om each other (F = 3.13, 937df; p = .044), and LSD post-hoc analysis showed reliable differences in the strength of the LVH bias between line-drawn and photographic face-sets (p<.023) as well as between line—drawn and realistic cartoon face-sets (p<.043), but not between photographic and realistic face- SCtS. One possible interpretation of the difference in strength of LVH bias between the line-drawn faces and other faces is that the usual right-hemisphere mechanisms were less aroused for the less face-like faces. Unfortunately, although each kind of face-set was collapsed over difliculty level for this analysis, there is no way to tell whether the line- drawn face-sets generally were more difficult to judge because task dificulty cannot be co-varied out of the analysis. The results nonetheless are suggestive and can be used in designing new studies of the independent contribution of facedness to the difficulty effect. Interpretation of Data from Other Studies How well do these hypotheses explain results fiom other face-perception studies that show a change toward (or reversal to) an RVH bias? The F eature-Analysis Hypothesis 55 0.75 1 3 .2 0.70 4 o .C 0 g 0.65 - d T o r .5 0.60 — 1: o o. 2 0.55 ~ 0. 0.50 -,_, ' ‘ ‘ ' fl photographic realistic line-drawn faces cartoon faces cartoon faces Experiments 6 and 7 Figure 6. Strength of LVH biases for photographic, realistic cartoon, and line-drawn cartoon face-sets for Experiments 6 and 7. The data collapsed over difficulty level, showing potential relation between strength of LVH bias and degree of “facedness.” Non-overlapping standard error bars indicate statistical reliability. accounts for results that show this change when subjects are cued to attend to facial features rather than to the whole face (Rhodes, 1985) and when subjects must discriminate between schematic faces that differ on a single feature (Patterson and Bradshaw, I975; Bradshaw and Sherlock, 1982; Fairweather, Brizzolara, Tabossi, & Umilta, 1982). It is less clear how it accounts for the effect when subjects are asked to make same-different judgments of two different views of the same face (Bertelson, Vanhaelan, & Morais, 1979). The Attenuation Hypothesis, on the other band, would seem to help to explain'the weakening of the LVH bias when faces are inverted and therefore look less like a face (Leehey, Carey, Diamond & Cahn, 1978; Hillis, Hiscock, & Rexer, 1995; Luh, 1997). As noted in the Introduction, however, none of these studies determined whether the weakened or reversed LVH bias was associated with an increase in subjective difficulty. 57 VII. CONCLUSIONS AND FUTURE QUESTIONS Diffictu and Cognitive Strategy Overall, the results suggest that when a visual judgment task becomes more difficult, subjects rely less on right-hemisphere strategies and more on left-hemisphere strategies. A major aspect of the change appears to be in the increased reliance on stimulus features and, to a lesser extent, on the use of a language-based strategy. Further studies are needed to assess the contribution of stimulus character (e.g., fitcedness). At this time, it also remains to be seen whether and to what degree these strategies are interrelated. Individual Differences Questions also remain about what the individual brings to the task as opposed to what the task itself elicits. For example, as discussed previously, individual differences in lateral specialization as indexed by handedness and, possibly, by the sex of the subjects my be related to lateralized hemispheric arousal and, perhaps, in processing style as well. The current experiments were not specifically designed to address these questions, and, with respect at least to the handedness variable, the number of lefi-handers was usually too small to permit meaningful analysis. Experiment 5 (with 281 right-handers and 57 left-handers) showed a difference in the overall strength of the bias, with right- handers showing the stronger LVH bias, which is consistent with the literature. In that Experiment, right-handers and left-handers showed virtually the same relation between bias strength and task difliculty, although the latter was not statistically reliable. Inspection of scores across the seven experiments, however, did not reveal any other 58 obvious trends in the data. The possible role of handedness in the further elucidation of the task difficulty effect thus remains for further research. By contrast, for comparisons of the sexes, the Ns were adequate. Only in Experiment 4, however, did men and women differ on LVH bias strength, with women showing a stronger overall LVH bias for MF face-sets. As noted previously, this difference was not pursued in the subsequent experiments so that data for men and women could be pooled for a more powerful analysis of the relation between task dificulty and LVH bias strength There was also an unexpected finding of a sex difference: for an indirect measure of task difficulty in Experiment 3 using 3-face sets and then for a direct measure of task difliculty in Experiment 5 using 2-face sets. In both cases, men rated the CFT as an overall more difficult task than did women. In neither case, however, were there sex differences in the relation between task difficulty and the strength of the bias. As discussed previously but not addressed directly in these experiments, there also is reason to believe tlmt a further role may be played by individual difi‘erences in characteristic style of hemispheric arousal independent of lateralized motor or perceptual dominance. Finally, individual differences in experience with any stimulus class or particular kind of test as well as learning history for the use of one or another processing style might be expected to contribute to a task being “easier” and therefore less likely to evoke a difficulty-related feature-search strategy. Like the other individual difi‘erence variables, the possrhle contributions of characteristic style of hemispheric arousal and experience/expertise remain for further research. 59 Attention-Arousal Model Assuming that attention is lateralized to the hemispace contralateral to the more aroused cortical hemisphere, a frmdamental tenet of the attention-arousal model, we can now characterize the direction of changes in hemispheric arousal when task difliculty changes. At this point, however, we can only speculate about changes in arousal within each hemisphere. For example, if there is a significant increase in left-hemisphere activity is introduced with increased task difficulty, are those newly aroused areas the homologues of the right-hemisphere areas, or are they different altogether? To the extent that left-hemisphere processing is associated with a different style of cognitive processing, the greater likelihood is that the areas would be different. For example, one would expect to find activation of left fiontal eye fields and of left parietal and subcortical structures associated with attentional control as well as some degree of activation of left-hemisphere fi'ontal and temporal language areas. Likewise, most normal adults being “face experts” so that all flee judgments are normally easy, one would nearly all face judgments to be processed predominantly in the right hemisphere, with the right posterior fusiform gyrus taking the lead, and with an LVH bias as a result. The firsiform gyms has been implicated for both expert processing and for flee processing (Gauthier, et al., 1997; Kanwisher, McDermott, & Chun, 1997; Puce et al., 1996), and it presumably will be active for any flee perception task — easy or dificult. The introduction of task difficulty is unlikely to recruit left-hemisphere mechanisms peculiar to an alternative strategy to the exclusion of fusiform activation. Therefore, to the degree that the introduction of a feature-search strategy has increased left-hemisphere arousal, it would not be expected to replace arousal in areas usually associated with flce 60 perception in normal subjects; instead it should activate areas that could introduce complementary processing strategies. One way to address all such questions and possibilities would be through real-time neuroirnaging studies of the concurrence of VH bias, difliculty level, and local arousal. Accuracy of Self-Report Functional imaging also could help to determine whether and to what degree subjects’ post-hoe reports of their strategies coincide with physiological changes found to be associated with the task difliculty effect. For instance, would subjects who reported talking to themselves for more difficult judgments show activation of known language areas, and would subjects reporting use of feature analysis show activation of areas known to be involved in feature searching (e.g., eye fields directing visual scanning, frontal areas associated with sequencing for point-to-point comparisons), or would their reports be unrelated to the strategies implied by actual patterns of arousal? Clinical Imflications Finally, the results have at least three kinds of implications for clinical practice. First, consider the interpretation of neruropsychological test results. As noted previously, insofar as task dimculty is progressively sealed in most neuropsychological tests, then flilures on more difficult items may reflect either a low initial level of skill or, based in part on the current research, the patient’s inability to shift strategies. Second, the possibility that task difficulty is related to cognitive processing strategy may help generate alternative hypotheses for the unusual co-occurrence of low-end errors with high-end success on some psychometric instruments (a pattern sometimes associated with test dissimulation). After an injury, a patient may show deficits for processing test items 61 in the usual way, while the ability to shift strategies remains intact. Being unable to process the easy items in the usual way, low-end errors are made fi'om the outset, but then a shift in strategy may occur, and fiom that point on test items are approached with the cognitive strategy usually reserved for more difficult items. Third, research in rehabilitation may help to determine, following an injury that has compromised a patient’s ability to shift strategies, whether shifting cognitive strategies for difficult tasks can be trained or cued. 62 APPENDIX A Photographic flee-sets fi'om booklets used for Experiments 6 and 7. In order from top left and fiom left to right, flee-sets shown are fiom pages I through 8, booklet A. Booklet B showed the same flee-sets in the same order but with positions A and B reversed. These eight face-sets are fi'om the pool of 34 photographic face-sets used in Experiment 5 and represent the four rated most diflicult (Experiment 6 and 7; pages 1, 3, 6, and 8) and the four rated easiest (Experiment 6 and 7; pages 2, 4, 5, and 7). 63 APPENDIX A (continued) M \ ., I I Z" ,_ . Si and J A I n /. ~l I 1.; A A . 193 9%. it? ‘ ‘ '\é‘; \ /®_ 8 I‘ . \ "I ll ”VIA A I A I A I A I A I A I ‘ ' A I A I A I Realistic and line-drawn flee-sets from booklets used for Experiments 6 and 7. Continuing in order from top left, and fi'om left to right, shown are face-sets from pages 9 through 23 fi'om booklet A. Booklet B showed the same flee-sets in the same order, but with positions A and B reversed. REFERENCES Benson, F. D., & Zaidel, E. (Eds) (1985). The Dual Brain, Hemispheric Specialization in Humans. New York: Guilford Press. Bentin, 8., Allison, T., Puce, A., McCarthy, G. (1996). Electrophysiological studies of flce perception in humans. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 8(6), 551-565. Bertelson, P., Vanhaelen, H., & Morais, J. (1979). Left hemifield superiority and the extraction of physiognornic information. In I. S. Russel, M. W. Van Hof, & G. Berlucchi (Eds), Structure and Function of Cerebral Commissures. University Park Press: Baltimore. pp. 400-410. (First publication 1979, by London: Macmillan.) Boles, D. B., (1979). Laterally biased attention with comment verbal load: multiple failures to replicate. Neuropsychologia, I 7, 353-361. Boles, D. B., (1987). Reaction time asymmetry through bilateral versus unilateral stimulus display presentation. Brain and Cognition, 6, 321 -333. Boles, D. B., (1989). Do visual field asymmetries intercorrelate? Neuropsychologia, 27(5), 697-704. Boles, D. B., (1990). What bilateral displays do. Brain and Cognition, 12, 205-228. Boles, D. B., (1994). An experimental comparison of stimulus type, display type, and input variable contributions to visual field asymmetry. Brain and Cognition, 24, 184-197. Bradshaw, J. L., & Nettleton, N. C. (1981). The nature of hemispheric specialization in man. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 4(1), 51-63. 65 Bradshaw, J. L., & Sherlock, D. (1982). Bugs and faces in the two visual fields: The analytic/holistic processing dichotomy and task sequencing. Cortex, 18, 211-226. Breier, J. 1., Simos, P. G., Zouridakis, G., Papanicolaou, A. C. (1999). Temporal course of regional brain activation associated with phonological decoding. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 21 (4): 465-476. - Bruyer, R., & Crispeels, G. (1992). Expertise in person recognition. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 30, 501-504. Bryden, M. P. & Allard, F. (1976). Visual hemifield differences depend on typeface. Brain and Language, 3, 191-200. Burton, L. A., & Levy, J. (1989). Sex differences in the lateralized processing of flcial emotion. Brain and Cognition, 11(2), 210-228. Carbary, T. J., Almerigi, J. B., & Harris, L. J. (1999). The left visual hemispace bias for the perception of composite flees: A test of the difliculty-of-discrimination hypothesis Brain and Cognition, 40, 1, 71—75. Carbary, T. J., Almerigi, J. B., & Harris, L. J. (2001). The left visual hemispace bias for the perception of chimeric flees: A further test of the difliculty-of-discrimination hypothesis Brain and Cognition, 46, 47-51. Carbary, T. J., Almerigi, J. B., & Harris, L. J. (2001). Task difficulty is related to lateral shifts in visual hemispace bias for judgments of emotion in chimeric flces Brain and Cognition. (in press). 66 Carlson, D. F ., & Harris, L. J. (1985). Perception of positive and negative emotion in fi'ee viewing of asymmetrical faces: Sex and handedness differences. Presented at the 13th Annual Meeting of the International Neuropsychological Society, February 6-9, 1985, San Diego, California. Diamond, R., & Carey, S. (1986). Why flees are and are not special: An effect of expertise. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 115(2), 107-1 17. Fairweather, H., Brizzolara, D., Tabossi, P., & Umilta, C. (1982). Functional cerebral lateralization: dichotomy or plurality? Cortex 18, 51-66. Farah, M. J. (1994). Beyond PET methodologies to converging evidence. Trends In Neurosciences, 1 7(12), 514-515. Farah, M. J., Levinson, K. L., & Klein, K. L. (1995). Face perception and within category discrimination in prosopagnosia. Neuropsychologia, 33(6), 661 -674. Farah, M. J., Wilson, K. D., Drain, M., & Tanaka, J. N. (1998). What is “special” about flee perception? Psychological Review, 105(3), 482-498. Farah, M. J., Tanaka, J. N., & Drain, M. (1995). What causes the flee inversion effect? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21 (3), 628-634. Gauthier, I., Tarr, M. J., Moylan, J., Anderson, A., & Gore, J. C. (2000). Does the visual subordinate-level categorization engage the functionally defined firsiform flee area? Cognitive Neuropsychology, 1 7 (1-3), 143-163. Gauthier, I., & Tarr, M. J. (1997). Becoming a “greeble” expert: Exploring the flee recognition mechanism. Vision Research, 3 7, 1673-1682. 67 Gauthier, I., Behrman, M., & Tarr, M. J. (1999). Can flee recognition really be dissociated fi’om flee recognition? Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 11(4), 349-370. Gauthier, 1., Williams, P., Tarr, M. J., & Tanaka, J. W. (1998). Training “greeble” experts: A fi'amework for studying expert object recognition processes. Vision Research, 38, 2401-2428. Gauthier, 1., Anderson, A. W., Tarr, M. J., Skudgarski, P., & Gore, J. C. (1997). Levels of categorizations in visual objects studied with fimctional MRI. Current Biology, 7, 645-651. Harris, L. J. (1992). Left-handedness. In I. Rapin & S. J. Segalowitz (Eds) Handbook of Neuropsychology, Vol. 6. (pp. 145-208). Amsterdam, Netherlands: Elsevier Science Publishing Co, Inc. Harris L. J. (1999a). Early theory and research on hemispheric specialization. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 25 (1), 11-39. Harris, L. J. (1999b). Personal communication, September, 1, 1999. Data from work by L. J. Harris, S. Pierce, & J. Henderson. Haxby, J. V., Grady, C. L., Horwitz, B., Salerno, J., Ungerleider, L. G., Mishkin, M., & Sehapiro, M. B. (1993). Dissociation of object and spatial visual processing pathways in human extrastriate cortex. In B. Gulyas, D. Ottoson, & P. E. Roland (Eds.), Functional Organization of the Human Visual Cortex (pp. 329-340). Oxford: Pergamon Press. 68 Haxby, J. V., Horwitz, B., Ungerleider, L.G., Masisog, J. M., Pietrini, P., & Grady, C. L. (1994). The functional organization of human extrastriate cortex: A PET-RCBF study of selective attention to flees and locations. Journal of Neuroscience, 1 4, 6336-6353. Hellige, J. B., & Webster, R. (1979). Right hemisphere superiority for initial stages of letter processing. Neuropsychologia, 1 7, 653-660. Hellige, J .B. (1993). Hemispheric Asymmetry: What ’s Right and What ’s Left. Cambridge; London: Harvard University Press. Hillis, S. K., Hiscock, M., & Rexer, J. L. (1995). Dual-task interference patterns reveal differential processing of upright and inverted flees. Brain and Cognition, 28, 1 55-1 72. Hoptman, M. J ., & Levy, J. (1988). Perceptual asymmetries in left- and right-handers for cartoon and real flees. Brain and Cognition, 8, 178-188. Irvy R.B., & Robertson, L. C. (1998). The Two Sides of Perception. Cambridge, MA: M.I.T. Press. Jaynes, J. (1976). The Origin of Consciousnes in the Breakdown of the Bicameral Mind. Boston: Houghton Miftlin Co. Kanwisher N., McDermott J., Chun M. M. (1997). The fusiform flee area: A module in human extrastriate cortex specialized for flee perception. Journal Of Neuroscience, 1 7 (11), 4302-431 1. Kinsboume, M. (1970). The cerebral basis of lateral asymmetries in attention. Acta Psychologica, Attention and Performance 111 (AF Saunders, ed.) pp. 193-201. North-Holland: Amsterdam. 69 Kinsbourne, M. (1973). The control of attention by interaction between the cerebral hemispheres. Attention and Performance IV. (S. Kornblurn, ed.) pp.239-256. New York and London: Academic Press. Kinsbourne, M. (1974). Lateral interactions in the brain. In M. Kinsbourne & W. L. Smith (Eds) Hemispheric Disconnection and Cerebral Function. pp. 239-259. Springfield, Illinois: Charles C. Thomas. Kolb, B., Milner, B., & Taylor, L. (1983). Perception of flees by patients with localized cortical excisions. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 3 7, 8-18. Kowner, R. (1995). Laterality in flcial expressions and its effects on attributions of emotions and personality: A reconsideration. Neuropsychologia, 33 (5), 539-559. Leehey, 8., Carey, S., Diamond, R., & Cahn, A. (1978). Upright and inverted flees: The right hemisphere knows the difference. Cortex, 14, 411-419. Levine S. C., & Levy J. (1986). Perceptual asymmetry for chimeric flees across the life- span. Brain and Cognition, 5 (3), 291-306. Ley, R. G., & Bryden, M. P. (1979). Hemispheric differences in processing emotions and flees. Brain and Language, 7, 127-138. Levy, J., & Kueck, L. (1986). A right hemifield advantage on a verbal flee-vision task. Brain and Language, 27, 24-3 7. Levy, J., Heller, W., Banich, M.T., & Burton, L. A. (1983). Are variations among right- handed individuals in perceptual asymmetries caused by characteristic arousal differences between hemispheres? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 9(3), 329-3 5 9. 70 Luh, K. (1997). Effect of inversion for perceptual bias for chimeric flees. Poster presented at the Ninth Annual Meeting of TENNET, 16-19 June, Montreal, (kuuukh. Luh, K. E., Rueckert, L. M., & Levy, J. (1991). Perceptual asymmetries for the free- viewing of several types of chimeric stimuli. Brain and Cognition, 16, 83-103. Luh, K. E., Redl, J. & Levy, J. (1994). Left- and right-handers see people differently: F rec-vision perceptual asyrmnetries for chimeric stimuli. Brain and Cognition, 25, 141-160. McCarthy, G., Puce, A., Gore, J. C., & Allison, T. (1997). Face-specific processing in the human fusiform gyrus. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 9(5), 605-610. Natale, M., Gur, R., E., & Gur, R. L. (1983). Hemispheric asymmetries in processing flcial expressions. Neuropsychologia, 21 , 555-566. Nakarnura, K., Kawashirna, K, Ito, K., Sugiura, M., Kat, T., Nakamma, A., Hatano, K., Nagumo, S., Kubota, K., Fukuda, H., & Kojima, S. (1999). Activation of the right inferior frontal cortex during assessment of flcial emotion. Journal of Neurophysiology, 82 (3), 1610-1614. Patterson, K., & Bradshaw, J. L. (1975). Differential hemispheric mediation of nonveflxflrnsualsfinnfli JburnalQflhquninunualIhychohmgy:Lhanan Perception and Performance, 1(3), 246-252. Posner, M. 1., Walker, J. A., Friedrich, F. J., & Rafal, R. D. (1983). Effects of parietal lobe injury on covert orienting of visual attention. Journal of Neuroscience, 4, 1863-1874. 71 Puce, A., Allison, T., Gore, J. C., & McCarthy, G. (1996). Face sensitivity regions in human extrastriate cortex studied by functional MRI. Journal of Neurophysiology, 74, 1192-1199. Renter-Lorenz, P. A., Kinsbourne, M., & Moscovitch, M. (1990). Hemispheric control of spatial attention. Brain and Cognition, 12, 240-266. Rhodes, G., & McLean, I. G., (1990). Distinctiveness and expert effects with homogeneous stimuli: Towards a model of configural coding. Perception, 19, 773-794. Rhodes, G. (1985). Perceptual asymmetries in flee recognition. Brain and Cognition, 4(2), 197-218. Rhodes, G., Brake, S., & Allison, A. P. ( 1993). What’s lost in inverted flees? Cognition, 47, 25-57. Rizzolatti, G, Buchtel, H. A. (1977). Hemispheric superiority to reaction time to flees: A sex difference. Cortex. 13, 300-305. Rizzolatti, G., Umilta, C., & Berlucchi, G. (1971). Opposite superiorities of the right and left cerebral hemispheres in discriminative reaction time to physiognomical and alphabetical material. Brain, 94, 431-442. Robertson, L. C., & Delis, D. C. (1986). “Part-whole” processing in unilateral brain damaged patients: Dysfirnction of hierarchical organizations. Neuropsychologia, 24, 363-370. Robertson, L., Lamb, M. R., & Knight, R. T. (1988). Effects of lesions of temporal- parietal junction on perceptual and attentional processing in humans. Journal of Neuroscience, 8(10), 3757-3769. 72 Rossion, B., Delvenne, J. -F., Dibatisse, Goffaux, V., Bruyer, R., Crormnelinck, M., & Guérit, J. -M. (2001). Spatio-temporal localization of the flee inversion effect: an even-related potentials study. Biological Psychology, 5 0, 173-189. Sergent, J., Ohta. S., & MacDonald, B. (1992). Functional neuroanatomy of flee and object processing - a positron emission tomography study. Brain, 115, 15-36. Sergent, J. (1991). Processing of spatial relations within and between the disconnected cerebral hemispheres. Brain, 114, 1025-1043. Tarr, M. (1999). Personal communication, September 2.\ Thompson, P. (1980). Margaret Thatcher: a new illusion. Perception, 9, 483-484. Trevarthen, C. (1972). Brain bisyrmnetry and the role of the corpus callosum in behavior and conscious experience. In I. J. Cernaeek & F. Podivinsky (Eds) Cerebral Interhemispheric Relations. pp. 319-333. Bratislava: Vydavatelstvo Slovenskej Akadémie Vied. Umilta, C., Sava, D., & Sahnaso, D. (1980). Hemispheric asymmetries in a letter classification task with different typefaces. Brain and Language, 9, 171-181. Wagner, N. M., & Harris, L. J. (1994). Effects of typeflce characteristics on visual field asymmetries for letter identification in children and adults. Brain and Language, 46, 41-58. Zurif, E. G., & Bryden, M. P. (1969). Familial handedness and left-right differences in auditory and visual perception. Neuropsychologia, 7, 179-187. 73 I14 IIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIII lillillllllilllllMlllliill 3 1293 02334 0031