r 1...-.. .... ....J&........~z . t) 0.16341 1/ a‘, Q I. «an... 3 1...... .{u .2 3.8mm. . in 25 x . 3.... . 2......- . . uranhnwiwflr 345...... 2191-... Mg. 51.1.3.9. a? ..o.ll:t-i utfl. .. . a: 2 2.! 4.1.22- 5.2.5:... . u 2 6:1!” . , , . iii]. J , . . , . . . “34...: 2...... . . . .. Ia . . 2 . 23:21.... . m... .91.... ,. hfiunfififiwgf . v .p m). t‘nl‘g . ::i:‘ l x . 5:34.... . 3.39.4 Tu... in... 5).."vG-‘r. ."s . I r :1..- ‘l:: . . .1:£..v}!.k!t‘.§3£.nifl... .3. ”Earn... 11.35....l52 t3! . . hmiili5lel 5101:. 3:32.... a .a. I. .515211 I: trill... $.13... Eh“: .. 95.. 7-13... 5... Fig,- 35.‘a$ tehmuwuifl Styli! -. : i . 1.. .11.: 11v}¢!l2.....! 13...: .82.! is. .131! {I‘lritii rt 5 r 11. .331; .6.“ _....w.......... .95: ilinlvli‘lti x \h 5.. z...- .3 y 9.. rit- I): égfifim s. .1. ¢°°L This is to certify that the dissertation entitled Police Social Capital and Officer Perfomnnce of Cmmmity Policing presented by Pmanda L. Robinson has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for Ph . D. . degree in Interdisciplinary Social Sciences Date W I 3/ 200/ MS U i: an Affirmative Action/Equal Opportunity Institution 0-12771 _ LIBRARY M'Chigah State University PLACE IN RETURN BOX to remove this checkout from your record. TO AVOID FINES return on or before date due. MAY BE RECALLED with earlier due date if requested. DATE DUE DATE DUE ' DATE DUE MAY 3 0 2005 JUL 1 4 2005 ”.3830 08 6/01 c:lClRC/DateDue.p65-p.15 POLICE SOCIAL CAPITAL AND OFFICER PERFORMANCE OF COMMUNITY POLICING By Amanda L. Robinson A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY College of Social Science 2002 ABSTRACT POLICE SOCIAL CAPITAL AND OFFICER PERFORMANCE OF COMMUNITY POLICIN G By Amanda L. Robinson Social capital is used as a theoretical framework to reveal the importance of networks of relationships between officers and their supervisors for performing community policing. Police social capital refers to the quality of officers’ relationships within the police organization; for example, with their peers and their supervisors. It is expected that officers with higher levels of social capital will be able to accomplish more community policing than their peers who have lesser amounts of this resource, controlling for officer characteristics and features of their work environment. Using data firom the Project on Policing Neighborhoods (POPN), two measures of community policing were developed: acts of community policing provided to citizens (comfort, referrals, and information) per citizen encountered, and time spent on community policing activities (attending community meetings, problem-solving, and crime prevention) per shift. Separate models were tested on these two measures to determine the relative influence of social capital (trust, cooperation, group cohesion, social support), officer characteristics (sex, race, education, tenure, assignment, training) and work environment (department, shift, beat problems, organizational support of community policing) on officer performance of community policing. Interaction models were also tested to determine the extent to which social capital interacts with characteristics of officers and features of their work environments. Results from Negative Binomial and Zero-Inflated Negative Binomial regression models did not support the central hypothesis of this research: social capital was not a significant predictor of either measure of community policing. Instead, work environment characteristics tended to offer more consistent explanations of community policing performance. Specifically, community policing varied significantly according to the department in which the officer worked, whether officers were assigned to be community policing specialists, and their levels of tenure. Implications of these findings are discussed in terms of their relevance to organizational factors which promote or hinder the implementation of community policing. This dissertation is dedicated to the beloved memory of James Eldridge Robinson, Minnie Lee Camp, and Jenny Lea Simpson. iv ACKNOWLEDGMENTS An ‘acknowledgements’ page implies the ability of one mere (ex)graduate student to remember and adequately address the multitude of personal and professional favors, advice, support, social interaction, love and faith that it takes from scads of family members, friends, peers, and mentors over many, many months for one Ph.D. to be completed. Given that disclaimer, I would like to mention a few people at Michigan State University that made my six years there especially interesting. My major advisor, Peter Kirby Manning, is a first-rate example of the positive and profound impact that a supervisor can have on a student. The unwavering concern and empathy he has shown for me and other students over the years speaks volumes about his character. Meghan Sarah Stroshine there over many beers and many years as a friend, co-author, proof-reader and editor extraordinnaire, and co-creator of thousands of confusing and usually incomplete SPSS data files. Long live Chanaman Industries! Many others at the school (or now elsewhere) deserve thanks for their continuous encouragement, energy, and good will over the years — Merry Morash, Christopher Damian Maxwell, Kevin Ford, Stephen Mastrofski, Christina DeJong, Kevin Gray, Sean and Tracy Varano, Sameer Hinduja, and Roni Mayzer. Finally I would like to extend my gratitude to those in my immediate family — Daddy D., Shirl, Mamasita, Pablo, Yebes, Matt #1 and of course the Eagle — whose courage and curiosity help me keep the academic life in greater perspective. TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES ...................................................... ix LIST OF FIGURES ..................................................... x INTRODUCTION ....................................................... 1 CHAPTER 1 REVIEW OF THE SOCIAL CAPITAL LITERATURE .......................... 5 The Sociological Significance of Social Capital .......................... 5 Historical Background ........................................ 5 Bourdieu’s Theoretical Framework .............................. 6 Existing Literature on Social Capital and Policing ........................ 9 Definition and Dimensions of Social Capital ........................... 11 Previous Measurement of Social Capital ............................... 12 Level of Trust .............................................. l3 Cooperative Exchanges ...................................... 14 Group Cohesion ............................................ 15 Social Support ............................................. 16 Police Social Capital .............................................. 17 The Impact of Social Capital ........................................ 19 Positive Outcomes .......................................... 19 Negative Outcomes ......................................... 22 CHAPTER 2 THE PHILOSOPHY AND PRACTICE OF COMMUNITY POLICING ............ 25 Community Policing as a New Police Mandate .......................... 25 Community Policing Activities ...................................... 27 Community Engagement ..................................... 28 Problem-Solving ........................................... 30 Providing Assistance to Citizens ............................... 31 Marginalization of Community Policing Within the Police Subculture ....... 33 CHAPTER 3 FACTORS AFFECTING PERFORMANCE OF COMMUNITY POLICING ........ 39 Police'Social Capital .............................................. 39 Features of the Officer’s Work Environment ........................... 40 Department ................................................ 40 Indianapolis, Indiana .................................. 41 St. Petersburg, Florida ................................. 42 vi Beat Characteristics ......................................... 43 Shift and Assignment ........................................ 44 Characteristics of the Officer ........................................ 45 Sex ...................................................... 45 Race ..................................................... 45 Education ................................................. 46 Tenure ................................................... 46 Training .................................................. 47 Moderated Causal Relationships ..................................... 50 Sex and Social Capital ....................................... 52 Race and Social Capital ...................................... 53 Education and Social Capital .................................. 55 Tenure and Social Capital .................................... 55 Work Environment and Social Capital .......................... 56 CHAPTER 4 DATA AND MEASUREMENT OF VARIABLES ............................ S9 The Project on Policing Neighborhoods ............................... 59 Description of Data Collection ...................................... 59 Systematic Social Observation ................................. 59 Structured Interviews of Officers ............................... 61 Sample ......................................................... 61 Measurement of Dependent Variables ................................. 63 Providing Acts of Community Policing to Citizens ................ 72 Time Spent Engaged in Community Policing ..................... 73 Description of Dependent Variables .................................. 74 Measurement of Independent Variables ................................ 77 Officer Characteristics ....................................... 78 Work Environment .......................................... 81 Social Capital Dimensions .................................... 84 Description of Independent Variables ................................. 89 CHAPTER 5 ANALYTIC MODELS AND METHODS ................................... 91 Analytic Models .................................................. 91 Additive Models: Assessing Direct Effects Among Variables ........ 91 Interactive Models: Testing Moderated Causal Relationships ......... 93 Analytic Methods ................................................. 98 Regression Models for Count Data ............................. 99 Negative Binomial Regression Model .................... 100 Zero-Inflated Negative Binomial Regression Model ......... 102 vii CHAPTER 6 RESULTS OF ANALYSES ............................................. 104 Bivariate Analyses ............................................... 104 Multivariate Analyses ............................................ 109 Additive Models ........................................... 109 Interactive Models ......................................... 111 Summary of Findings ............................................. 117 CHAPTER 7 ISSUES RAISED BY THE RESEARCH FINDINGS ......................... 120 Substantive Issues ............................................... 120 Research Questions Revisited ................................ 120 Leadership ............................................... 125 Organizational Structure .................................... 128 Geographic Responsibility ............................. 128 Decentralization ..................................... 129 Methodological Issues ............................................ 130 Interaction Terms .......................................... 130 Regression Equations ....................................... 132 Causal Order ............................................. 133 Sampling Strategy ......................................... 134 Measurement of Community Policing .......................... 135 Measurement of Social Capital ............................... 136 APPENDIX A CORRELATION MATRD( OF ALL VARIABLES ........................... 138 REFERENCES ....................................................... 141 viii Table 1 Table 2 Table 3 Table 4 Table 5 Table 6 Table 7 Table 8 Table 9 Table 10 Table 11 Table 12 Table 13 Table 14 LIST OF TABLES Summary of Expected Direct Relationships. ...................... 49 Measurement and Descriptive Statistics for Dependent Variables. ..... 75 Measurement and Descriptive Statistics for Officer Characteristics. ................................... 79-80 Measurement and Descriptive Statistics for Work Environment. ...................................... 82-83 Measurement and Descriptive Statistics for Social Capital Dimensions ........................................... 84-85 Principal Component Factor Analyses of Social Capital Dimensions. ............................................... 88 Measurement and Descriptive Statistics for Interaction Terms. ....... 96 Bivariate Correlates of Community Policing Variables. ............ 105 Bivariate Correlates of Community Policing Variables - Interaction Terms. ......................................... 108 Additive Models for Community Policing Variables. .............. 110 Interaction Models - Zero-Inflated Negative Binomial Regression on Community Policing Time. .................. 1 13-114 Interaction Models - Negative Binomial Regression on Community Policing Acts. ............................... 115-1 16 Summary of Findings. ...................................... 117 Correlation Matrix of All Variables ........................ 138-140 ix LIST OF FIGURES Figure 1 Conceptual Model. .......................................... 51 INTRODUCTION Social capital is expected to provide an important new perspective on the social organization of policing within the new era of community policing. Like employees in other work organizations, police employees rely on work relationships for information, access to opportunities, and support to increase the likelihood of productivity. In this research, the term “social capital” is used to refer to the quality of officers’ relationships with their peers and their supervisors. Dimensions that are important to these relationships include the level of trust, the frequency of cooperative exchanges, the level of group cohesion, and the amount of social support. Police officers’ work relationships are considered to be a resource (if social capital is high) or a barrier (if social capital is low or not present) affecting how often officers will perform community policing activities. In short, it is expected that police officers are able to draw upon their social capital in order to “get things done.” Community policing activities may be especially dependent on police social capital, as this new policing movement is substantially marginalized within the traditional police subculture. Investigating the relationship between social capital and the likelihood that officers will engage in community-oriented activities can provide us with both a broader and deeper understanding of police behavior in the community policing era. The application of the concept of social capital (a sociological term) to the field of criminology has resulted in research that primarily focuses on the social capital of communities, and how this is an important resource resulting from strong police- community partnerships (a major tenet of the community policing philosophy). For example, Greene (1998) refers to the importance of measuring the “changes in the frequency, duration, and quality of police and community interactions” and “public service networks created through such efforts” (p. 150). Referring to the quality and quantity of police-community relationships can be considered an implicit reference to social capital. Although various efforts made by the police and/or community actors to enhance social control are dependent in part upon levels of social capital, as of yet no one has examined the levels of social capital among police officers. If we do not know the distribution of social capital among police officers, and the barriers preventing and resources promoting its utilization, then our methods of encouraging strong police- community partnerships will remain limited. This could have dire consequences for the success of community policing initiatives, which have in recent years been embraced by the public, police administrators, many police scholars, and the federal government. Criminal justice research on police behavior would benefit from the theoretical perspective of social capital. Limited research exists that incorporates a theoretical perspective which can help us understand and predict officer engagement in many police activities, particularly community policing. This issue becomes even more salient as the police are being evaluated by more audiences in more different ways than ever before. As expectations of police performance expand, so should our knowledge of the theoretical underpinnings that guide their behavior. Only then will we be able to understand why certain officers perform in certain ways, suggest how to facilitate better outcomes from officers engaged in community policing activities, and describe the types of relationships IQ that most effectively increase officers’ stocks of social capital. Several questions are answered by the current study: (1) Is social capital related to how police officers perform their jobs? Specifically, what is the relationship between levels of social capital and officers’ engagement in community policing activities? (2) What is the relative contribution of police social capital in a model that also includes characteristics of the individual officer and their work environment? (3) Do officer characteristics, such as their sex, race, education, or tenure moderate the relationship between social capital and community policing? (4) Do features of officers’ work environment, such as their department and their perceptions of the department’s support for community policing, moderate the relationship between social capital and community policing? Although there is an extensive body of sociological literature on social capital, there is substantial room for improvement. Many researchers have not specifically defined what they mean by the concept “social capital,” explained their measurements of this construct, or been able to simultaneously incorporate all of the variables that may influence the social capital-outcome relationship they are investigating. Using limited measures of social capital also has precluded a full understanding of this complex construct. Additionally, the majority of the studies in this area have been qualitative in nature, usually involving in-depth accounts with small samples or a case study approach. While informative and particularly important in laying the theoretical groundwork, the social capital literature suffers from a lack of research on sample sizes large enough to be quantitatively tested. This study could make a valuable theoretical contribution by empirically testing a model of police social capital, helping us specify which relationships are important to the theory and which are not. Furthermore, in his monograph on the “state of the state” of social capital research, the noted social capital researcher Portes (1998) concluded that, “the greatest theoretical promise of social capital lies at the individual level” (p. 21). This study moves social capital research forward by specifically _ defining the term, providing multiple measures of the different dimensions of social capital, testing a model which includes all of the variables that could potentially affect the police social capital — community policing relationship, and providing this information at the officer-level. A final reason why this research is important is that only a handfiil of police scholars has linked the social capital idea to policing, and they have only done so in an indirect or tangential way. This concept is rich enough to be the primary focus in a study of police behavior using detailed observational and survey data. Only then will we be able to assess the true value of employing a social capital framework to the study of policing. CHAPTER 1 REVIEW OF THE SOCIAL CAPITAL LITERATURE The Sociological Significance of Social Capital Historical Backgound The term that has come to the forefront of current sociological research, social capital, was originally introduced by French sociologist Pierre Bourdieu in the late 1970's. He made distinctions between four types of capital: (1) economic, (2) cultural, (3) social, and (4) symbolic (Bourdieu, 1986). The first type corresponds with material goods or wealth, and its relationship to the human condition has been under investigation since the inception of sociology. The second type, cultural capital, refers to goods such as art, language, or books which are proxies for “the long-lasting dispositions of the mind and body” (Bourdieu, 1986, p. 243). Bourdieu used this term to understand the differences in educational attainment of French children originating from different social classes, as these outcomes were hypothesized to be a function of the cultural capital possessed by the family. Specifically, when children’s cultural capital mirrors the dominant form of cultural capital in society (i.e., upper—middle class), scholastic achievement is greater. Thus, the generative nature of capital was revealed by Bourdieu, as the educational system reproduces larger social structures in society that favor certain groups over others. The third form of capital, social capital, was developed by Bourdieu to give a name to the resource present within communities or groups that facilitates collective action. Bourdieu defined social capital as “the aggregate of the actual or potential resources which are linked to possession of a durable network of more or less institutionalized relationships of mutual acquaintance and recognition” (p. 248). Bourdieu’s definition of social capital contains two elements: (1) the network of social relationships which a person can mobilize, and (2) the volume of capital (economic, cultural, social, symbolic) possessed by the network members. Social capital, then, represents the quality of social relationships that can be linked to various outcomes. Bourdieu’s fourth form of capital is symbolic capital, which refers to all types of capital once they are perceived as legitimate. In other words, ‘Vvhen the possession of any kind of capital is justified not only in the eyes of those who benefit most from its distribution, but also in the eyes of those who are most deprived of it” (Peillon, 1998, p. 218). Symbolic capital confers “the power to create the official version of the social world” (Mahar, Harker, & Wilkes, 1990, p. 13). Symbolic capital may be equated with legitimacy and prestige. An important commonality of Bourdieu’s four types of capital is their conceptualization as inherently positive and productive; in his view, people strive to increase their stocks of all four types of capital. Bourdieu’s Theoretical Framework To fully comprehend the importance of Bourdieu’s contribution to our understanding of the social world, it is first necessary to put his forms of capital into a broader context. Just as paper money only has value because it can be exchanged, substituted, or transformed into tangible goods and services in society, concepts of capital must also be recognized above and beyond their intrinsic value. Capital is just one element in Bourdieu’s theoretical framework. He sought to make a contribution that would provide a balance to the (often mutually exclusive) sociological traditions of individualism and structuralism. He provided the concept of afield to identify areas of social space where people struggle for position. Their struggle, and resulting position, is affected by the distribution of various forms of capital. Refening to an earlier example, the French educational system was considered by Bourdieu to be afield, and the children in it actors whose struggle and outcome varied according to their levels of cultural capital, which were dependent on those of their families. Evident in this example is not only the force of social structure to pattern actions, but also the potential of individuals to fight for position, to use their agency to the best of their advantage. Bourdieu used the term habitus to explain the process by which individuals are molded by social structure; it is a mediating construct between social structure or fields and people struggling in them for capital. So what is habitus? It can be thought of as a person’s world view -- the knowledge, beliefs, and dispositions that are produced by socializing agents such as parents and also the social world in which they exist. As Bourdieu (1990) explained, habitus consist of “schemes of perception, thought, and action” (p. 54), or on a more basic level, “things to do or not to do, things to say or not to say” (p. 53). But all habitus are not created equal. As “the internalization of extemality,” (Bourdieu, 1990, p. 55) we can see that some extemalities have more resources and support compared to others, which affects the development of habitus. “Habitus is intimately linked to capital in that some habitus (those of dominant social and cultural factions) act as multipliers of various kinds of capital, and in fact constitute a form of capital (symbolic) in and of themselves” (Mahar, Harker, & Wilkes, 1990, p. 12). Habitus may be mental perceptions or attributes, but in the real world, in hierarchical societies, these also translate into differences along lines of class, race, and gender. Bourdieu conceptualized habitus as generative; that is, they tend to generate, promote, or reproduce themselves. Returning to the school example, Bourdieu (1984) stated that school “transforms social classifications into academic classifications, with every appearance of neutrality” (p. 387) in part because it is patterned on the habitus of the upper-class. In short, fields are not level, capital is not evenly distributed, and your habitus counts for a lot. The interaction between these various concepts provides a foundation for understanding the behavior of individuals and groups in society. We are all embedded in fields of struggle, seeking to acquire and exchange various forms of capital, constrained by the limits of our habitus (or, conversely, propelled if our habitus is consistent with that of the dominant group in society). The strategies we develop and incorporate into this “game” of positioning and struggle in various fields are referred to by Bourdieu as practice. Capital, as one feature of this game, is an important mechanism which facilitates the style, content, and success of practice. As the title of Bourdieu’s (arguably) most important book, The Logic of Practice (1990) indicates, these actions and strategies are not random, but patterned. Because there is an underlying logic to our practice, we can expect or predict certain behaviors or outcomes based on the knowledge we have about the field under investigation and the distribution of capital among the players of the game. Existing Literature on Policing and Social Capital Although the literature linking social capital to the study of policing is limited, several scholars have joined these two fields, with promising results. One of the first to do so was Manning (1994), who incorporated one of Bourdieu’s four forms of capital, symbolic capital, into his theoretical discussion of the police response to domestic violence. He provided a critical outlook on the exchange of capital in policing fields, arguing that “police arrests in domestic conflicts are seen by some as enhancing symbolic capital by ‘empowering women’... [but] for other observers, it creates yet another intrusion of the state into private relations, an enforcement of class-biased notions about disputing, and a source of exacerbation of conflict and increased costs to lower—class domestic units” (p. 86). Because the police reflect and reproduce the habitus of the upper-classes, their symbolic capital is used to reinforce “the patriarchal order and class- biased character of policing” (Manning, 1994, p. 89). Consequently, he contended that police work tends to reduce the capital of the lower-classes. Lyons (1999) used the concept of social capital in his study of the Seattle police department to illustrate his point that the contemporary community policing movement is not achieving its intended aims. He provides the following explanation of the relationship between social capital and community policing, “the most basic reciprocal exchange at the heart of stories about community policing is a police/state commitment to perform their duties in a way that enhances the generation of social capital in communities and a community commitment to invest a portion of that capital in cooperative efforts with the police to improve public safety” (p. 28). Lyons (1999) is critical of community policing because its proponents operate under the assumption that these new police strategies are supposed to help communities reclaim lost social capital and use it to improve local, informal social control, yet this assumption is routinely violated. Instead, the police mandate is broadened as the fight continues “to control political, economic, and social resources for the power to say what policing is and who communities are” (Lyons, 1999, p. 4), usually at the expense of the very communities the community policing movement was originally intended to benefit. Duffee et al. (1999) also touch upon the importance of community social capital for the success of community policing initiatives. As they stated, “without sufficient social capital... policing initiatives to prevent crime in such areas are particularly problematic — ofien engendering no citizen involvement at all or increasing, rather than reducing, dissension within the neighborhood” (p. 94). Although Henig (1982) found that poor perception of police services was related to declining local organization, Duffee et al. (1999) maintain that the plight of such neighborhoods needs to be understood in terms of a larger “urban struggle,” where the police are just one institution that should play a role in “constituency building” (p. 94). Overall, this literature tends to focus on the social capital of communities, and how it pertains to their relationships with the police. The one exception is a recent study by Pino (2001)-who specifically examined social capital and community policing in a small department in Iowa. Employing a qualitative methodology, he examined interactions among and between the police, the citizenry, and neighborhood groups. He 10 found that there was a substantial lack of trust between the public and the police, as well as between community policing and regular patrol officers, and that this had a detrimental effect on efforts at co-production of safety. This study, while restricted in generalizability due to the methodology and sample, points to the importance of understanding police relationships and their subsequent impact on the success or failure of community policing initiatives. Definition and Dimensions of Social Capital In the sociology literature, social capital refers to relationships among individuals, networks of relationships, and people’s “ability to mobilize a wide range of personal social contacts” (Newton, 1997, p. 577) to accomplish a particular objective. Coleman (1988) was one of the first to apply this concept to sociology in America. He extended Bourdieu’s merger of two streams of thought: (1) a sociological focus on the norms, rules and obligations which socialize people and subsequently govern their behavior, allowing for action to be explained in a social context, and (2) an economic focus on the independent and rational goals of people which they subscribe to purely out of self- interest. As Bourdieu explicated in his idea of habitus, neither of these views adequately explains the social world, for people are not just blank slates scribed upon by society, nor are their actions completely independent of the social context in which they occur. By “introducing social structure into the rational action paradigm,” Coleman (1988, p. S95) made a valuable application of social capital to contemporary sociological thought in the United States. While research on social capital has suffered due to ambiguous definitions and 11 poor operationalization of this construct, several themes emerge in the literature that I refer to as dimensions of social capital. These include the level of trust, the frequency of cooperative exchanges, the level of group cohesion, and the amount of social support present in relationships. That is, researchers have either used all or part of these dimensions to explain the formulation and/or utilization of social capital in various settings. The dimensions relevant to this study are discussed below.1 Particular attention is paid to how these dimensions have been measured in past research, with implications for how these dimensions are measured in the current study (discussed in detail in Chapter 4). Previous Measurement of Social Capital The empirical research on social capital often includes measures of the number of relationships as a proxy for social capital (Bursick, 1999; Burt, 1997; Coleman, 1988; Frank & Yasumoto, 1998; F urstenberg & Hughes, 1995; Granovetter, 1973; McCarthy & Hagan, 1995; Molinas, 1998; Robinson & Morash, 2000; Teachman, Paasch, & Carver, 1997; Wellrnan & Wortley, 1990). Because numbers alone tell us nothing about the quality of the relationship or the potential of relationships to be a social resource for those in the relationship, other research (less often empirical) has described social capital not 1 Civic engagement, for example, has often been referred to as an important component of social capital in community—level research. This has been measured as the proportion of people volunteering in various religious, social service, or community-based associations (see Greeley, 1997; Portney & Berry, 1997; Furstenberg & Hughes, 1995; Brehm & Rahn, 1997). While this dimension is important for macro-level research on social capital (or research on social capital at the individual-level in a different context), civic engagement is not an important dimension in my study of police social capital and its affect on officer performance of corrrrnunity policing activities. It would be important to address, however, if the focus of the current study was on community social capital and its impact on community participation in corrrrnunity policing initiatives. 12 only in terms of the number of social relationships, but also in terms of the qualities present in these relationships which may enhance or constrain the potential of those relationships to be a resource. The literature has identified level of trust, cooperative exchanges, group cohesion, and social support as important qualities to assess when studying social capital. In other words, it is assumed that pe0ple who have relationships that are high in these qualities have more social capital than people whose relationships do not possess these qualities. Level of Trust People’s level of trust, whether it is in a generalized form, in each other, in a particular group, or in a government, has been an important dimension of the social capital construct. Fukuyama (1995), for example, puts trust as central to his definition of social capital: “social capital is a capability that arises from the prevalence of trust in a society or certain parts of it” (p. 26), as does Molinas ( 1998): “social capital is defined here as the level of trust and community networking” (p. 413). Research at the micro- level has also found trust to be a central issue in how people create and maintain their levels of social capital. Specifically, the norms and values of individuals or, “those cultural values and attitudes that predispose citizens to cooperate, trust, understand, and empathize with each other” (Newton, 1997, p. 576) have been studied in relation to social capital by many researchers. Coleman (1988) alludes to these as ‘social norrns’; for example, the “norm that one should forgo self-interest and act in the interests of the collectivity” (p. S104). Foley and Edwards (1997) mention the “attributes of individuals which favor their civic engagement” (p. 551). Woolcock (1998) refers to “information, 13 trust, and norms of reciprocity” (p. 153) that inhere within relationships. Portes (1998) describes the internalized norms of trust and reciprocity that are necessary in the formation of social capital; similarly, Schrnid (1999) mentions “an internalized sense of obligation and ethical norm” (p. 3). Despite the wealth of literature on this subject, past measurement of this dimension is lacking for two reasons. For example, some researchers have used the General Social Survey’s questions on generalized trust as proxies for social capital, when these questions are not context-specific. For example, these questions ask respondents, “Do you think most people would try to take advantage of you if they got a chance, or would they try to be fair?” and “Generally speaking, would you say that most people can be trusted or that you can’t be too careful in dealing with people?” It would be more fruitful to ask whether a person trusts a specific person, place, or thing. Second, the social capital construct is more than “just trust.” Improved measurement of this construct would include many dimensions of social capital. Cooperative Exchanges Social capital researchers ofien refer to “norms of reciprocity,” which when present in social relationships increase the potential of those relationships to be a resource. The logic underlying this dimension of social capital is that this type of norm makes people give back in exchange for taking. After an exchange occurs (whether it is money, material goods, information, or emotional aid like support or advice), it is understood by both parties that the exchange will be paid back at a later date. This is a form of trust in itself; trust in the belief that cooperation is beneficial and that exchanges 14 will be reciprocated. Past researchers have tapped into “norms of reciprocity,” or what I term cooperative exchanges, by looking at patterns of giving and receiving in a community (Hofferth & Iceland, 1998), or analyzing actions one person in a relationship took that helped the other person maintain or acquire certain resources (Frank & Yasumoto, 1998). Others have tapped into this dimension by asking respondents questions such as, “How often do you and people in your neighborhood do favors for each other?” and “When a neighbor is not at home how often do you and other neighbors watch over their property?” (Sampson, Morenoff, & Earls, 1999). The cooperative exchanges dimension could also be measured with questions tapping into how often people share particular goods, or the level of cooperation within a particular group, such as a family, neighborhood, work group, or community agency. Group Cohesion Because social capital research is often done at the community-level, researchers have been interested in what makes groups cohesive. It is assumed that cohesive groups, or groups that have members who are supportive or trustworthy of each other, who share norms, and/or have similar beliefs, will have more social capital. Measurement of this dimension can be as basic as the proportion of residents in a particular neighborhood that are friends or acquaintances, the frequency that a group engages in social activities, or the amount of people in a group that simply like each other (Sampson, 1991; Sampson, Morenoff, & Earls, 1999; Bursick, 1999). Social ties that have emotional density, for example, with a high level of mutual confiding and intimacy, also increase social capital 15 (see Granovetter, 1973). Norms about particular behavior also influence the performance of that behavior; for example, Coleman (1988) found that whether mothers expected their children to attend college affected whether they actually did. Cohesiveness has also been measured by questions assessing similarity among group members. Bursick (1999), for example, asked people whether they agree with the statements “I have a lot in common with people in my neighborhood,” and “The people in my neighborhood are a lot like me.” The underlying assumption of this dimension is that groups that “get along” and share similar beliefs and characteristics will have more social capital than those that are antagonistic or whose members share very different beliefs or values. Social Support This dimension of social capital has been closely tied to the actions of people in a social relationship that help one member accomplish a particular goal. For example, F urstenberg and Hughes (1995) examined the support given and received in a mother- child dyad, and found it related to the child’s successful school outcomes. Other researchers have investigated different types of social support in relationships, such as financial, emotional, and providing services, and found that the type of support is often a function of the type of relationship (e. g., whether the relationship is between friends, family members, neighbors, etc.) (Wellrnan & Wortley, 1990). In short, this dimension is usually measured in a particular context, such as the family, workplace, or community. When social support is high, positive outcomes are more likely, and when it is low, these outcomes are more difficult for the actors to obtain. 16 16 M. Upo SUP} Police Social Capital In the present study, I am specifically concerned with police relationships with their peers and supervisors, and how these impact officers’ performance of community policing activities. Patrol work is considered to occur “in the context of territorially based work groups” (Klinger, 1997, p. 283). This territoriality affects officers’ attitudes and actions, depending on which work group they occupy in the department (Reuss-Iarmi, 1983) (e. g., community policing, patrol, SWAT, or special gang-suppression or street- crimes units). Assigning officers to work together in a geographically defined area creates the opportunity for informal group norms to arise; they are considered to be far more important in governing officer behavior “on the streets” than are departmental regulations or police management (Klinger, 1997; Reuss-Ianni, 1983). Examples of these group norms include “watching out” for one’s partners and the rest of the officers working on the same shift and beat, not “sucking up” to supervisors, and other variations of solidarity and loyalty (Crank, 1998; Reuss-Ianni, 1983). Group norms about appropriate levels of activity are also important modifiers of police behavior because they tend to “discourage innovation while they encourage the status quo” (Manning & Van Maanen, 1978, p. 267; also see Rubinstein, 1973). Past research has substantiated the importance of work groups in policing. An officer who does not conform to informal norms about what constitutes “real” police work and how to accomplish these tasks, for example, may not be fully included in his or her work group. Officers who cannot draw upon relationships that are rich in terms of trust, cooperative exchanges, and social support and/or who are not members of cohesive groups (i.e., they are excluded or 17 colic; 1116 V; peer 1 relati pmg 18%] Mg; marginalized from these groups), will have additional obstacles to overcome than officers who benefit from work relationships that are rich in social capital. Miller’s (1999) in-depth study of Neighborhood Policing Officers (NPOs) provides some insight into the importance of police-peer relationships in the community policing era. Specifically, she found that NPOs who assertively established relationships with beat officers experienced “greater understanding and cooperation from their colleagues” (p. 109). Although the community policing movement has drawn attention to the value of police relationships, we have not specifically examined the role that police peer relationships play in performance, and if and how it varies according to what policing tasks are being performed. Quality relationships with supervisors also occupy an important place in police work. Social capital theory identifies people with decision-making authority, such as supervisors, as “targets” who may be especially important contributors to one’s stock of social capital (Wood, 1997, p. 599). Officers rely on supervisors for information, support, and evaluations of their performance (Van Maanen, 1983). Positive relationships between officers and supervisors are so vital to efficient police work that programs specifically designed to increase positive interaction between the ranks have recently been suggested (Beck & Wilson, 1997). It is also important to remember that supervisor support is considered vital to the success of innovative community-oriented police activities (Geller & Swanger, 1995; Goldstein, 1990; Skogan & Hartnett, 1997). Without supervisor support the implementation, as well as instrumental success, of these programs is considered unlikely. 18 Miller (1999) documents how supervisor support allowed NPOs to overcome much of the stigma associated with performing community policing tasks (considered by many officers to be “social work” or “women’s work”; i.e., not real police work). Indeed, in the department studied by Miller (1999), many upper-level management positions were held by former NPOs; this had a legitirnizing effect on the entire community policing program. Police social capital, then, may be more important to officers who are deemed to occupy marginalized roles within the police organization. Specifying these relationships becomes especially salient given the implications for performing community-oriented policing tasks. The Impact of Social Capital Research usually tends to link social capital to positive outcomes; however, the term is much more encompassing and flexible than this value judgment would imply. Accordingly, recent research has been critical of inherently benevolent views of social capital (e.g., that originally formulated by Bourdieu). As Foley and Edwards (1997) noted, “its uses may range from asocial to antisocial to broadly prosocial” (p. 552). This section summarizes the findings from research framing social capital in a positive light, followed by research that looks at its “dark side.” Epsjtive Outcomes Much of the social capital literature refers to inherently positive yet ambiguous outcomes (i.e., civic virtue, quality of life, etc.), but empirical support for these benefits boils down to two variables: education and crime. Specifically, social capital has been shown to increase positive educational outcomes (Coleman, 1988; Teachman, Paasch, & 19 Carter. 1 ample flow fro: than educatic disadtar p035°55é it is irrel amount. concept you’ll. ' enhancs 00mm Socioec. has ten Kfil'acl Sampsc 3008] c COmmu Char 3C1: C(”men Carver, 1997; F urstenberg & Hughes, 1995). Teachman, Paasch, and Carver (1997), for example, viewed social capital as a “filter through which human and financial capital flow from the parents and the community to the child” to produce improved educational achievement. Similar to Bourdieu, Coleman (1988) used social capital to explain the educational achievement of children growing up in economically and socially disadvantaged communities. As he stated, “if the human capital [skills or knowledge] possessed by parents is not complemented by social capital embodied in family relations, it is irrelevant to the child’s educational growth that the parent has a great deal, or small amount, of human capital” (p. 8110). Furstenberg and Hughes (1995) also used the concept of social capital to explain the successful outcomes of a sample of disadvantaged youth. They defined social capital as a “resource upon which individuals may draw to enhance their opportunities” (p. 581). Their research suggested that family-based and community-based social capital played an important role in helping youth overcome socioeconomic disadvantage. Most of the research employing a social capital framework to the study of crime has revealed a negative relationship: as one increases the other decreases (Bursick, 1999; Kawachi, Kennedy, & Wilkinson, 1999; Kennedy et al., 1998; Sampson, 1995, 1997). Sarnpson’s (1997) work on juvenile delinquency, for example, led him to conceptualize social capital as a “buffer” against the negative effects of high levels of delinquency in the community. As he stated, “in a system involving parents and children, communities characterized by an extensive set of obligations, expectations, and social networks connecting the adults are better able to facilitate the control and supervision of children” 20 (1997, p. 52). Positive relationships among community members are key, and can be partly facilitated by: (1) organizing supervised leisure-time for youths, (2) observing and reducing street-comer congregation, and (3) establishing mentor relationships between adults and youth, and also building adult acquaintances within communities (Sampson, 1995, p. 210). Thus, to build social capital in neighborhoods and communities is to build barriers against crime and violence. Other researchers have also posited a beneficial relationship between social capital and crime rates. Using national state-level data, Kennedy et a1. (1998) found support for their hypothesis that low levels of social capital were related to firearm homicide and violent crime. They measured social capital using indicators from the General Social Survey (GSS) such as levels of trust and civic engagement among community members. Their results supported a path model whereby income inequality (relative deprivation) decreased stocks of social capital, which in turn increased crime rates. Later work conducted by the same authors (Kawachi, Kennedy, & Wilkinson, 1999) tested similar conceptual fi'amework using state-level ecological data. In addition to GSS trust variables, they also included a single-mother household variable as an indicator of social capital. This measurement of social capital was negatively related to state levels of violent crimes as well as property crimes. The authors took this as evidence supporting Sampson’s work on social disorganization theory; specifically, “crime is... a mirror of the quality of the social environment” (Kawachi, Kennedy, & Wilkinson, 1999, p. 719). 21 Negative Outcomes The majority of the literature either explicitly or implicitly refers to social capital as a positive feature of social life, but other research offers several examples of the negative consequences of social capital (Portes, 1998; Portes & Landolt, 1996). First, the same strong social relationships that are necessary for the formation of social capital within a group may also serve to exclude new members. When new members are excluded, so are new sources of social capital, and the resulting isolation may result in the group’s downfall. In his analysis of neighborhood security, Hope (1998) pointed out the positive (e. g., membership, natural surveillance, etc.) as well as negative (e. g., exclusion, stagnation, etc.) effects of having a closed community structure. Waldinger’s (1995) analysis of social capital in the New York City construction business, for example, led him to conclude that, “social structures such as closure or network multiplexity may generate social capital for insiders while also excluding outsiders from the resources that social capital generates” (p. 560). Referring to Granovetter’s (1973) seminal work, these communities lack the benefits which accrue from weak ties. Second, a highly cohesive group (i.e., one with a dense social network) may prevent the success of its members. This may result from suict demands for conformity which restrict group members’ personal freedoms and subsequently encourage them to leave the group. For example, tight-knit communities such as the Amish, while benefitting from the social capital that rich networks of relationships produce, also suffer from the exodus of young people from their community. Wilson’s (1987) work on the plight of America’s inner-cities also provides evidence of the detrimental impact that may 22 occur to communities that experience a departure of people, skills, and resources. Third, group solidarity may foster an “us versus them” mentality which in the long-term discourages successful initiatives and dissolves group cohesion. Referred to as “downward leveling norms,” these norms may take the place of “mainstream” norms, especially in communities traditionally marginalized, stigmatized, or victimized by society at large (Portes, 1998, p. 17). For example, “‘wannabes’ -- the latest lexical contribution of inner-city youth to mainstream culture -- are those who imitate the ways and lifestyles of the majority in search of success. Often, these efforts only meet scorn from fellow members of their community, who see them as a threat to solidarity and their own sense of self-respect” (Portes & Landolt, 1996, p. 21). Few researchers have provided empirical evidence revealing the “dark side” of social capital. One exception is the research conducted by McCarthy and Hagan (1995), who incorporated notions of embeddedness (Granovetter, 1973), social capital (Coleman, 1988), and differential association (Sutherland, 1942) to explain onset of criminal activity. Specifically, they proposed that “embeddedness in networks of deviant associations provides access to tutelage relationships that facilitate the acquisition of criminal skills and attitudes, assets we call ‘criminal capital’” (p. 63). Analyzing rates of drug-selling, theft, and prostitution among a sample of homeless youth, they found evidence supporting their hypothesis; the acquisition of criminal capital led to detrimental consequences because the number and length of relationships increased criminal activity. Sirrrilar to the relationship between social capital and entrance into the mainstream world of work, criminal capital was related to entry into the underground world of criminal 23 work. Although limited research exists on the negative consequences of social capital, it is still apparent that to equate the concept with only positive outcomes would be inaccurate as well as unnecessarily limiting. Conversely, taking a value-neutral stance with regard to social capital allows the researcher to investigate all the potential outcomes with which it may be associated. As it is doubtful that the outcomes discussed above are the only ones related to levels of social capital, it would seem constructive to broaden the research agenda. Policing as a public good can be considered an important outcome of social capital that requires investigation. How levels of police social capital affect the distribution of these “goods,” especially in a community policing context, is the focus of the current research. The next chapter examines the literature on community policing, the activities encouraged by this policing philosophy, and its place within the police subculture. 24 CHAPTER 2 THE PHILOSOPHY AND PRACTICE OF COMMUNITY POLICIN G Community Policing as a New Police Mandate Common wisdom points to three events which precipitated the policing reform movement known as “community policing.” First, civil unrest during the 19605 challenged police legitimacy and brought questionable police practices into the national spotlight. Second, recognition of the isolation of the police from the public led to interest in citizens being “co-producers” of police services. That is, police and community members should share responsibility for crime reduction and work together toward meaningful, long—lasting change. Third, the community policing movement arose out of the ashes of research findings that constituted a “systematic demolition” of the assumptions underlying the professionalism movement (Walker, 1984). As research indicated that “nothing works,” reformists attempted to identify and adopt policing strategies that might make a difference (e. g., foot patrol, permanent beat assignments, mini-stations, etc). The philosophy of community oriented policing is currently widespread and embraced by many citizens, police administrators, scholars, and local and federal politicians. For example, the 1994 Crime Act authorized $8.8 billion for community policing programs, with the result that almost 90% of Americans have community policing officers working in their communities (U .S. Department of Justice, 1999). The 25 under 15102 ll noxidnals. atomplist. variation in community cities. rese. programs 1 programs 1' attempted elements c 35 com u Ht (20111311111; POllClllg 1 Policing l underlying premise of the community policing philosophy is that the police and various individuals, agencies, organizations, or community groups should work together to accomplish mutual goals. At the implementation level, however, there can be substantial variation in the tactics and strategies deployed by police departments as part of a community policing program. For example, in a study of community policing in eight cities, researchers concluded that there was more difference than similarity among the programs (Grinc, 1994; Sadd & Grinc, 2000). Some of these community policing programs focused on aggressive street enforcement and drug crackdowns while others attempted community organizing and interagency cooperation. There are several elements or categories of activities that are commonly recognized, and widely accepted, as community policing that are discussed in the next section. However, given the wide variety of police activities subsumed under the community policing “umbrella,” it may be helpful to first document what community policing is not (see Trojanowicz, Kappeler, & Gaines, 2001, p. 18-27). Community policing is not a technique, it is a philosophy. Community policing is not public relations, it is a substantive change in the police-public relationship. Community policing is not soft on crime, it is “smart” on crime. Community policing is not flamboyant, it achieves results through steady, long-term efforts. Community policing is not paternalistic, it must empower officers and citizens in order to achieve results. Community policing is not an independent entity within the department, it is a philosophy that must inundate the entire department. Community policing is not cosmetic, it requires that the department make substantial changes in how it deals with the community. 26 Community policing is not just another name for social work, it recognizes the fact that the majority of police work involves non-crime related duties. Community policing is not elitist, and special efforts need to be made to counteract hostility from general patrol officers who might hold this belief. Community policing is not designed to favor the rich and powerful, it is an egalitarian view that promotes providing assistance and support to citizens of all jurisdictions. Community policing is not a panacea, it will not fix all problems but will be more effective at addressing problems than traditional policing. Community policing is not “safe,” but officers have to be trusted enough to take risks and make mistakes. The next section describes police activities that are generally considered to reflect a community policing philosophy, and the categories of activities operationalized as community policing in the current research. Community Policing Activities Reflecting the diversity and reality of police work, Trojanowicz and Bucqueroux (1992) identified 18 duties inherent in the role of officers engaged in community policing: law enforcement, directed patrol, community involvement, identifying and prioritizing problems, reporting, problem-solving, organizing, communicating, conflict resolution, referrals, visiting, recruiting and supervising volunteers, proactive projects, targeting special groups, targeting disorder, networking with the private sector, networking with non-profit agencies, and administrative/professional duties. The multitude of activities considered a reflection of the community policing philosophy can be grouped into three general categories: (1) police engagement of the community in the production of order, (2) a proactive response by police to community problems, for example using a problem- 27 solving strategy, and (3) use of the broadened police role to more frequently provide general assistance to citizens. These community policing activities are discussed in the sub-sections below, although it must be recognized that in practice these categories would not necessarily be mutually exclusive. One example from California demonstrates the overlap between different elements of community policing: problem-solving and providing assistance to citizens. A police sergeant established a new domestic violence protocol for his department after analyzing crime data that revealed that the domestic incidents tended to escalate over time, as well as comprising a significant proportion of calls for service. The new protocol mandated that officers make personal contact with the victim within one week of the incident, and again after one month. The purpose of these follow-up visits was to provide victims with general assistance and information, and to provide referrals to appropriate agencies in the community. To assess whether the problem was being solved, additional crime data were analyzed. Results showed that calls for service decreased 57% from 1996 to 1997 in the “hot spot” domestic viOlence locations identified by the department (Sampson & Scott, 1999). Commqu Engagement This theme of the community policing philosophy emphasizes an expanded police presence in communities in order to facilitate community capacity to exercise social control. As Rosenbaum (1998) stated, “the challenge for police today and into the 21St century is to find creative ways to help communities help themselves” (p. 14). In other words, police are no longer simply expected to enforce the law but to provide a broad 28 array of services aimed at increasing safety and order within communities. The underlying premise guiding this expansion of the police role is that the police cannot solve community problems without the help of citizens and corrrrnunity agencies. Community policing advocates propose that the police and the public ought to become “co-producers” of public safety, each contributing to the maintenance of law and order, because “together, police and public are more effective and more humane co-producers of safety and public order than are the police alone” (Skolnick & Bayley, 1988, p. 1). The police must, therefore, engage the community in order to build a productive, meaningful, working partnership. For example, community policing officers could attend meetings with various community groups and associations to open channels of dialogue, ideally leading to the identification of community problems and the creation of strategies for their solution. In Chicago, “building bridges between police and community members” was vital for the success of community policing (Skogan & Hartnett, 1997, p. 110). Beat meetings were how the department was able to convey to the community that the new policing philosophy was a long-term strategy intended to stay. Unlike some community policing initiatives in other departments, in Chicago the beat meetings were held regularly, at various locations, and were attended by the officers patrolled the beats. These meetings were an opportunity for community members to raise concerns to police officers, for the participants to identify and work together to solve problems, and to exchange information. As the authors conclude, “people can participate only where there are opportunities to do so” (Skogan & Hartnett, 1997, p. 160). Community engagement is 29 thus a particularly important aspect of any community policing project. Problem-Solving One part of the community policing philosophy that concerns improved policing is that police should not only respond in a reactive mode to crime and disorder, but should also work in a proactive way to address these issues. Problem solving has been recognized as a central characteristic of community policing departments because it uses community input to identify crime problems and determine the appropriate strategies to address them. To put it bluntly, “community policing without problem solving is not community policing” (J olin & Moose, 1997, p. 291). As opposed to the traditional strategy of random or preventive patrol, whereby police hope to decrease crime and disorder by their mere presence, problem-solving is a strategy police use to fight specific crimes with specific plans (Goldstein, 1990). Eck and Spehnan (1987) developed the widely accepted and used SARA model of problem—solving, which identifies four stages of the problem-solving process: (1) scanning to collecting information to identify a crime problem, (2) analysis to determine the nature and extent of the problem, (3) response through the creation of a specific strategy to address the problem, and (4) assessment to determine whether the response alleviated the problem. For example, officers engaged in problem-solving would attempt to prevent the occurrence or recurrence of particular problems, and develop plans or projects that go beyond merely responding to a particular call in order to address the underlying cause of the problem. In contrast to traditional, reactive policing, the focus of police effort within a problem-solving model is on the underlying condition — when that 30 is addressed then it is likely that calls for service will decrease to a significant extent. One frequently cited study documenting the success of a problem-oriented approach took place in Newport News, Virginia (Eck & Spelrnan, 1987). Police were inundated with burglary incidents originating from a particular apartment complex. Officers surveyed residents about this particular crime problem but also learned that the physical state of the complex was of major concern to residents. Information gleaned from other city agencies (e. g., the fire department, public works, etc.) confirmed that physical deterioration was a serious issue and directly contributed to the burglary problem (i.e., aged window and door frames that were rotting made break-ins easy to commit). Having a clearer picture of the underlying condition helped police create a long-term strategy for decreasing burglary incidents. Officers worked with the apartment manager and city agencies to improve the physical state of the buildings. A neighborhood association was formed with help from police that was able to successfully lobby for continued upkeep of the complex. Due to these efforts, the burglary rate decreased by 35%. Providing Assistance to Citizens The community policing reform emphasizes a broad, social role for the police, with the goal of police becoming more responsive to citizen concerns. Also referred to as “personal service,” and following the trend in the private sector of putting “customers first” or “listening to customers” (Skogan, 1998, p. 162), this philosophical dimension aims to build trust and positive interactions between the police and the community they serve (Cordner, 1998). No longer are police to be viewed solely as gatekeepers to the 31 criminal justice system; they are being called upon to monitor the turnstile to social service and government agencies as well. Some goals of providing citizens with assistance, information, and support include: alleviating citizen fear about particular problems in the community; garnering citizen support for police initiatives to solve problems; educating citizens about their vulnerability to crime; and helping citizens solve problems for themselves (Goldstein, 1990). Guided by a community policing philosophy, the police serve as instigators and motivators for cooperation between agencies with the goal of creating networks of services that benefit citizens. Community policing officers, therefore, are encouraged to provide citizens with needed assistance and information, including referrals to other community agencies that might be better suited to handle the citizen’s problem. Some departments have implemented “swaps” where agency workers and police officers spend time in each other’s work environment to better learn how to assist citizens (Goldstein, 1990). In addition to the usual gun and radio, community policing officers might also be dispatched to calls armed with lists, contact information, and descriptions of services provided by local community agencies. Miller’s (1999) ethnography on community policing officers in one police department suggests that they are often able to provide citizens, and abused women in particular, access to more types of assistance compared to when they solely respond to calls for immediate help. Compared to their traditional 911-driven counterparts, “[t]he nature of the neighborhood position encouraged [community policing] officers to become actively involved with the community they served: in prevention programs, case follow- 32 ups, working on continuing problems, and acting as liaisons with residents, businesses, city services, and the criminal justice system” (p. 183). Consequently, community policing officers often had more information about the citizens they served, which they could then use to provide referrals, informally monitor problem citizens, and provide reassurance, advice and support to citizens, and victims especially. Marginalization of Community Policing Within the Police Subculture While the community policing philosophy has been warmly embraced by the public, many police scholars and administrators, and the political establishment, it often has encountered resistance from the rank-and-file officers who must ultimately translate this complex and multifaceted concept into concrete policing strategies and tactics. Several reasons exist for this less-than-enthusiastic acceptance by officers are often cited in the community policing literature. First, it-has often been unclear to officers what is meant by the term “community policing.” Officers may be able to understand and even admire the concept in the abstract, but difficulty arises when they have to actually put this philosophy into practice (Goldstein, 1990; Kelling & Moore, 1988; Sadd & Grinc, 2000). A corrrmon refrain has been “what exactly do you want us to do?” and until recently, police supervisors, scholars, and administrators were unable to adequately answer this question. Second, officers often have been expected to “do” community policing in addition to other traditional police duties such as patrolling and responding to calls for service. A lack of resources (particularly training) has often made the burden of this new reform 33 movement fall primarily on the shoulders of those least-equipped to handle it (Williams & Sloan, 1990; Zhao, Thurman, & Lovrich, 2000). Lurigio and Skogan (2000), for example, found that patrol officers were significantly less confident with their ability to engage in community-oriented policing than their higher-ranking counterparts, although patrol officers are the police employees expected to actually engage in community policing. The above two issues facilitate patrol officer resistance to community policing initiatives, which in turn facilitates the marginalization of community policing within the police subculture. Even when resources and training are devoted to community policing initiatives, resistance from officers is still encountered. For example, in a research design involving officer surveys before and after the implementation of a department-wide community policing strategy in Chicago, the conclusion reached was that “the bulk of the officers in the field had not yet ‘got the message’ or committed themselves to the program in a significant way” (Skogan & Hartnett, 1997, p. 105), despite a significant level of department-wide training. The first round of training involved an initial orientation and a 3-day skill building session of patrol officers. Supervisors were then given a 4-day training curriculum comprising nine community policing modules, and were responsible for conducting the second round of training during roll—call training sessions which covered the nine community policing topics. This training made it clear that community policing was to be a “real” program rather than a “paper” program and was in the department to stay. A subculture is commonly defined as the attitudes, norms, and beliefs systems 34 adopted by employees to make sense of their work environment. The police subculture has been identified as the single largest barrier facing those who want to implement community policing (Sparrow, Moore, & Kennedy, 1990). To be successful, researchers suggest that community policing initiatives “be compatible with the existing culture and organizational climate in a department and with the basic concerns and needs of police personnel” (Lurigio & Skogan, 2000, p. 255). Unfortunately, the components of the traditional police subculture and the community policing philosophy are often at odds with each other. The police subculture rests on themes of uncertainty, danger, violence, suspicion, and coercive authority — often leading to an “us versus them” mentality regarding police relationships with citizens, and increasing the likelihood that officers adopt work group norms of loyalty and solidarity (see Bittner, 1970; Crank, 1998; Manning, 1997; Skolnick, 1997; Westley, 1970). These norms often put them at odds with management and other “outsiders” and make officers likely to resist change and protect the status quo. Officers derive honor and status (and reduce uncertainty and degradation) from their official mandate which is to enforce the law. Departmental selection, training, reward, and promotion systems also reinforce the supremacy of law enforcement within the police subculture. The end result is that law enforcement is viewed as the only “real” police work. The police subculture also reinforces traditional notions of masculinity, with “real” police work being “men’s wor ”; all other policing activities are viewed as not “real” police work, and therefore the responsibility of female officers or social workers. 35 scppor police produ e cre doing office C0111: The community policing philosophy directly challenges many of the norms and values underlying the traditional police subculture. For example, community policing supports cooperation and trust between police and citizens, in contrast to the traditional police view of the citizenry as a hostile enemy to be distrusted instead of engaged in a productive partnership. Within a community policing context, officers are encouraged to be creative and solve problems rather than to just “lay low” and “cover their ass” by doing the bare minimum to avoid potentially negative attention from the public, other officers, and their supervisors (see Van Maanen, 1978). Others have noted that the community policing philosophy represents the “feminization” of police work, by valuing stereotypical female qualities such as communication, cooperation, and supportive interpersonal relationships (Miller, 1998). The community policing reform movement, therefore, poses many threats to the officers whose cultural values have rested on themes of masculinity, danger, suspicion, and violence. To handle this new threat to the traditional police identity, there is evidence to suggest that officers have marginalized community policing (and the officers who practice it) within the police subculture. Wesiburd and McElroy (1988) found that when given the choice, community policing officers in New York continued to choose policing strategies that had a traditionally high status within the police subculture, such as aggressive law enforcement. The officers who practice community policing are often derided by general patrol officers as not doing “real” police work (Pate & Shtull, 1994); the traditional police tactics of patrol, surveillance and arrest. Moore (1992) epitomized the view of community policing officers within the police subculture with his observation 36 that “they became known as ‘grin and wave’ squads and ‘rubber gun’ squads” by the regular patrol officers (p. 135). Marginalization may also arise from resentment related to practical issues such as staffing and resources. Regular patrol officers may perceive community policing officers to be “wasted resources” insofar as their assignments involve a lowered expectation to engage in traditional policing tasks; the “slack” from CPOs falls on the shoulders of patrol officers. One patrol officer reflected this sentiment when he expressed that community policing officers should be “arrested for theft when they pick up their paychecks” (Pino, 2001 , p. 209). The pronounced lack of trust and respect noted in many departments has, not surprisingly, translated into negative work experiences for community policing officers. Winfree and Newbold (1999), for example, found that community policing officers perceived less supervisor support than regular patrol officers. In their national assessment of community policing implementation, Zhao, Thurman, and Lovrich (2000) identified internal organizational impediments as the most significant obstacle to successful implementation (more significant, for example, than community impediments such as citizen resistance or a lack of local government support). The items comprising the “organizational impediments” factor included: resistance from middle-management, line-officer resistance, departmental confusion about what community policing is, problems in line-level accountability, officer’s belief that community policing is “soft” on crime, a lack of community policing training, and union resistance. The two items that had the highest factor loadings were middle-management and line-officer resistance. The authors concluded by suggesting that any long-lasting 37 organizational change toward community policing must correspond with a change in the values and norms underlying the police subculture. While there is some evidence to suggest that the traditional police subculture might be changing (see Haarr, 1997; Paoline, Myers & Worden, 2000), it still unfortunately presents a major challenge to community policing initiatives. The next chapter discusses three categories of factors hypothesized to influence the likelihood that officers will engage in community policing activities: (1) police social capital, (2) work environment, and (3) officer characteristics. 38 CHAPTER 3 FACTORS AFFECTING OFFICER PERFORMANCE OF COMMUNITY POLICING Police Social Capital Past literature has revealed the important of police work groups on police behavior, and there is no reason to expect these relationships to be less important in the community policing era. Research has documented that officers marginalized or excluded from their peer group (e. g., because they are of a minority race or are women) have suffered a lack of acceptance, a denial of needed information, sponsorship, and promotion opportunities (Buzawa, 1981; Ellison & Genz, 1983; Holdaway & Barron, 1997; Martin, 1980; Milutinovich, 1977). These issues can subsequently affect their work experiences, performance, and advancement within the police organization. Although not previously or explicitly stated as such, what makes certain officers marginalized is their lack of social capital. Officers who lack social capital in their work environments face higher hurdles and bigger barriers to getting the job done than their counterparts who are embedded in productive, supportive, and trustworthy work relationships. It is expected that officers who have relationships with peers and supervisors that are rich in social capital will be more productive than officers without this resource, who may not have the same level of opportunity or support to engage in various community policing activities. Community policing officers, therefore, might 39 particularly need relationships that are strong in terms of trust, c00perative exchanges, group cohesion, and social support to accomplish a type of policing not wholeheartedly accepted within the police subculture. While it is hypothesized in the current study that the social capital dimensions will be positively related to the amount of time an officer spends engaged in community policing, the social capital literature suggests that negative outcomes may also result. If officers who have high levels of social capital are found to be significantly less likely to spend time on community policing activities, this could be interpreted as an example of the “dark side” of social capital. For example, officers rich in this resource might be better able to circumvent departmental dictates supportive of community policing. In this case, the support, cooperation, trust and group cohesion officers have in their work units and/or with their supervisors could be used to cover up poor community-policing performance or Shirk community-oriented activities, or to further other (possibly negative) policing outcomes not included in this study. Despite this possibility, the central hypothesis of the current study is that as levels of social capital increase, so will the likelihood that officers engage in community policing activities. Features of the Officer’s Work Environment Department The available evidence on the two departments included in this study suggests that their work environments might differ in important respects relevant to community policing, such as their interpretation of this policing philosophy. For example, one department (Indianapolis) takes a “broken window” aggressive order maintenance 40 6“ approach, with the police chief emphasizing traditional’ law enforcement activity” (Mastrofski et al., 2000, p. 317) while the other department (St. Petersburg) emphasizes building positive police-citizen partnerships (Paoline, Myers, & Worden, 2000). Furthermore, a greater proportion of officers in St. Petersburg are assigned as community policing specialists (22% compared to 5% in Indianapolis) (Mastrofski et al., 2000). This departmental difference could impact the frequency with which officers engage in community policing activities, resulting in St. Petersburg officers performing more community policing activities. Additional information about the two departments is provided below. Indianapolis, Indiana. The jurisdiction of the Indianapolis Police Department (IPD) is referred to as the Police Services District, 3 portion of Indianapolis-Marion County for which the department is responsible. At the time of the POPN study, the IPD served a population of more than 377,000 people. The UCR Index Crime was 100 per 1,000 residents and 37 per officer (Parks et al., 1999). In the years 1996-1997, the department employed about 1,000 full-time sworn officers, about half of which were assigned to patrol. The sworn force was 83% male and 79% white (Parks et al., 1999). The geographic responsibility for the Indianapolis Police Department was divided among four patrol districts: North, West, East, and South. Within each district, officers were assigned to one of five shifts: Day (5:00 am to 2:00 pm), Day Tact (9:00 am to 5:00 pm), Middle (1 :00 pm to 9:00 pm), Late Tact (7:00 pm to 3:00 am), and Late (10:00 pm to 6 am). These shifts were staggered so that shifts overlapped when service needs were high. Officers’ and supervisors’ work schedules were determined by their assignment to 41 one of three work schedules with rotating days off (referred to as A, B, or C “letter days”). A work squad consisted of the officers and supervisors assigned to the same district, shift, and letter day. The department implemented community policing in 1992. The Deputy Chiefs of each patrol district had considerable autonomy and latitude in determining the day-to-day operation of their districts. This resulted in wide variation in the organization and practice of community policing across districts. For example, in the West district the community policing strategy took an aggressive order maintenance approach. Conversely, the focus of community policing in the North district tended toward “community building” (Mastrofski, Worden, & Snipes, 1995) than aggressive order maintenance; for example, officers were encouraged to positively interact with community members. Community policing in the East district was practiced by a special unit and had a problem-solving focus. Finally, no community policing was practiced in the South district. St. Petersburg, Florida. Just over 240,000 residents inhabited St. Petersburg at the time of the POPN study. While St. Petersburg has a smaller population, its UCR Index Crime Rate is similar to that of Indianapolis, with 99 per 1,000 residents (Parks et al., 1999). The violent crime rate in St. Petersburg was more than three times the national average: about 2,250 violent crimes per 100,00 residents compared to 716 (Bureau of Justice Statistics, 1995). The St. Petersburg Police Department (SPD) employed about 500 full-time sworn officers, and similar to the IPD, about half (n=283) were assigned to patrol. The majority of officers were white (78%) and male (87%) (Parks et al., 1999). Officers working in St. Petersburg are deployed in four shifts: Day (7 am to 3 42 pm), Evening (3 pm to 11 pm or 4 pm to 12 am), 4th Relief (7 pm to 3 am), and Midnight (11 pm to 7 am or 12 am to 8 am). The department’s geographic responsibility is divided into three districts (North, South, and West). The SPD implemented community policing in 1990. Each of the three districts is responsible for a “zone” of the department’s 48 Community Policing Areas (CPAs). CPAs are analogous to the concept of a patrol beat; it is the smallest unit of geographic responsibility. A zone consisted of three sectors, with each sector representing a conglomeration of CPAs. At the time of this study, the SPD had 63 community policing officers (CPOs), over twice that of the IPD even though the department is half the size. In St. Petersburg, the permanent, geographic deployment of officers who focused on community-building with neighborhood organizations (Parks et al., 1999) resulted in a more uniform organization and practice of community policing compared to Indianapolis. The available evidence regarding the organizational contexts of the two departments suggests, therefore, that [PD officers will probably engage in less community policing compared to SPD officers. It is also reasonable to suggest that officers’ perceptions of how supportive their department is of community policing efforts will also affect their proclivity to engage in community policing. Beat Characteristics Regarding the primacy of territorial knowledge, Rubinstein (1973) stated that an officer “combines his knowledge of local behavior with his conceptions of how the public streets are used to analyze and perform many of his routine obligations” (p. 151). An officer’s assigned beat has been found to impact his or her level and type of activity (Klinger, 1997; Smith, 1986). The conclusion by some scholars that community policing tends to work the least where it is needed the most (i.e., in poor, crime ravaged, socially 43 disorganized and minority communities) also points to the profound impact that community or beat characteristics may have on whether community policing goals are accomplished (Skogan & Hartnett, 1997; Walker, 1999; Williams & Murphy, 1990). Officers who work in beats that have a significant amount of major crime problems (such as drug dealing, theft and burglary, or vandalism) might have less time to engage in community policing activities than their counterparts working in less troubled areas. As such, it is important to include officers’ perceptions of beat problems in a model predicting community policing performance. Shift and Assigment Recent research has investigated performance differentials between community policing officers and 911-responders. Although the study conducted by Mastrofski et a1. (1995) did find a difference in arrest rates, only 1 of the 17 variables examined differed to a statistically significant degree between the groups. Robinson and Chandek (2000a) failed to find a significant difference between community policing and traditional units when handling domestic violence calls. Recently, however, DeJong, Mastrofski, and Parks (2001) found that community policing officers spent more time engaged in problem-solving activities than did officers assigned to general patrol. Because the dependent variable in this study is officer engagement in community policing activities, it is important to include officer assignment (community policing versus general patrol officer) as a control variable, since theoretically community policing officers might be expected and given the resources to accomplish more community policing activities. Similarly, officers working the day shift would be expected to have more Opportunity for 44 community policing activities because most citizens (and citizen groups) are awake and functioning during this time. Characteristics of the Officer While most research finds very little difference in the performance of male and female officers, performance differences might emerge when we start to measure non- traditional policing activities, such as those guided by a community policing philosophy. For example, DeJong (2000) found that female officers are more likely to provide comfort to citizens than their male counterparts, and Hale and Wyland (1999) report that female officers may communicate better and subsequently de-escalate potentially violent situations. Although the evidence is limited, it is reasonable to believe that female officers might more frequently engage in community policing activities. Ric; Research suggests that an officer’s race is not an important variable to consider when measuring performance with traditional indicators such as making arrests or using excessive or deadly force (see F yfe, 1981; Reiss, 1968). To conclude that minority officers and white officers are identical, however, may be misleading. Mastrofski (1981) found that black officers were more knowledgeable of local citizen organizations in black neighborhoods. In Chicago, it was found that minority officers were significantly more optimistic about community policing than their white counterparts (Lurigio & Skogan, 2000), and although we cannot assume that attitudes are always consistent with behavior, it may be the case that racial differences emerge when we investigate non-traditional 45 police activities, such as corrrrnunity policing. It is therefore expected that nrinority officers will engage in more community policing than their white counterparts. Education The relationship between levels of education and police performance is less straightforward. While there is no evidence to suggest that college educated officers behave differently on the street (Sherman, 1978), more recent research finds that performance improves as education increases. For example, college educated officers may receive fewer complaints compared to their less educated counterparts (Kappeler, Carter, & Sapp, 1992). Researchers who followed a cohort of officers for ten years found a positive relationship between college education and supervisor ratings of job lmowledge (Truxillo, Bennett, & Collins, 1998). Kakar ( 1998) found that officers with some college or a college degree reported performing better, and Palombo (1995) found that they were more professional. The relationship between officer education and officer performance warrants further investigation, but it is likely that as education increases so would the skill and ability necessary for officers to engage in community policing activities. Mute Most research tends to find that as years of experience increase, the amount of arrest activity decreases (Binner, 1990; Muir, 1977; Stalans & Firm, 1995). Roberg, Crank and Kuykendall (2000) also report that younger officers tend to work harder and be more productive than older officers. The effect of tenure on community-oriented performance indicators has only recently been studied. DeJong (2000), for example, found that tenure improved the likelihood that female officers would provide comfort to 46 citizens. Conversely, more experienced officers were found to spend less time on problem-solving than their less experienced counterparts (DeJong, Mastrofski, & Parks, 2001). The available evidence, therefore, provides a conflicting account of the relationship between tenure and community policing. However, the present study assumes a relationship that has been supported by the majority of research, that tenure will decrease activity, in this case community policing. 1% Officers who have received more training on how to perform community policing activities might be expected to spend more time engaged in these activities, due to an increase in ability (and perhaps confidence) in how to perform community policing. Although DeJong, Mastrofski, and Parks (2001) did not find community policing training to significantly increase the amount of time an officer spends problem-solving, others contend that training is the key to successful implementation of community policing (Glensor & Peak, 2000; Zhao, Thurman, & Lovrich, 2000). It is therefore expected that as the amount of training an officer has received on community policing increases, so will the likelihood that he or she will perform community policing activities. Table 1 presents a summary of the direct relationships tested in the current study. The focus of this research is on the link between social capital and community policing. It is expected that all four dimensions of social capital (trust, cooperation, group cohesion, and social support) will significantly increase the likelihood that officers engage in community policing. Additionally, because officers are constrained by features of their work environment, it is expected that officers who are members of the IPD, who 47 are not assigned to community policing, who work at night, and who perceive a high level of problems in their assigned beat will engage in less community policing. Officer characteristics are hypothesized to play a small role in explaining community policing performance, with the exception that community policing training is expected to be an important predictor of community policing performance. Overall, police social capital and characteristics of the officer’s work environment are expected to exert the strongest effects, while officer characteristics will exert a relatively weak influence on community policing. 48 Table 1 Summgy of Expected Direct Relationships. Variable expected to impact officer Direction of expected Magnitude of expected performance of community relationship. relationship. policing. Social Capital Level of Trust positive strong Cooperative Exchanges positive strong Group Cohesion positive strong Social Support positive strong Work Environment Characteristics Department (Indianapolis) negative strong Day Shift positive strong Community Policing Assignment positive strong Beat Problems negative strong Department Support of CP positive strong Officer Characteristics Sex (female) positive weak Race (minority) positive weak Education positive weak Tenure negative weak Community Policing Training positive strong 49 Moderated Causal Relationships Research has documented relationships between social capital and the characteristics of individuals; however, we are still far from drawing blanket conclusions about these relationships. Because the extant research implies a strong possibility that stocks of social capital will vary according to different individual and organizational factors, included in the analytic plan are tests of moderated causal relationships, also known as interaction terms. The relationship between social capital and community policing is expected to vary depending on certain officer characteristics and features of their work environment (i.e., officer characteristics and work environment variables will moderate the relationship between social capital and community policing) (see Figure 1). Past studies that examine how social capital varies according to different individual and organizational characteristics are reviewed below. 50 Figure l Concpptual Model. OFFICER CHARACTERISTICS SOCIAL CAPITAL COMMUNITY POLICIN G WORK ENVIRONMENT 51 Sex and Social Capital A person’s sex has been shown to covary with his or her level of social capital, but the evidence suggests that information about the context in which these ties are located is vital, as men may have an advantage in workplace networks, whereas women may excel in familial or community networks (Hofferth & Iceland, 1998; Molinas, 1998; Moore, 1990; Rountree & Warner, 1999; Wellrnan & Wortley, 1990). Specific to the study at hand, Haarr’s (1997) research in a police patrol bureau found that officers tended to interact most frequently with their same race-gender group. Martin (1980) found that female officers avoided many interactions with peers and supervisors as a result of these interactions being misconstrued as involving a sexual component. Because females are already marginalized both numerically and within the traditional police subculture, having fewer interactions with officers from other units or shifts compounds their disadvantage. If female officers are not incorporated into workplace networks to the same degree as their male colleagues, then their stocks of social capital would be lower since they are removed from relationships that could provide them with support, information, cooperation, and access to opportunities (Martin, 1980). Although women might have relationships of a higher quality (e. g., involving more trust, support, etc.), due to their small numbers in policing (especially in positions where they hold power, such as supervisors), this may not be sufficient for overcoming the likelihood that they will have lower stocks of social capital than their male peers. Since police departments, like all human organizations, incorporate societal notions and expectations related to gender, male and female officers probably have stocks of social capital that differ to a significant 52 extent. Race and Social Capital It is not unreasonable to believe that in a society where race is related to many important variables, such as crime, poverty, and health, that it would not also be related to social capital. Sociological research investigating this relationship has revealed its complex nature. For example, while some researchers have not found a significant relationship between race and levels of social capital (Antonucci et al., 1998), others have found that compared to whites, minority persons and communities tend to have less social capital (Bursick, 1999; Edwards & Foley, 1997; Portney & Berry, 1997; Sampson, 1997). Brehm and Rahn (1997) discovered a significantly negative relationship between race and generalized trust, considered to be a component of social capital because it affects civic engagement. Waldinger’s (1995) ethnographic research into the construction trade found that ethnic enclaves could produce both positive and negative manifestations of social capital. On the one hand, close racial/ethnic work groups fostered trade and cooperation among minorities, but on the other hand these same relationships were detrimental for minorities trying to acquire the skills and connections necessary for success in predominantly white fields. In other words, minority contractors created their own networks apart fi'om the ‘old boys’ network,’ which both helped and hurt them. A notable exception to this trend of social capital racial differences is found in research conducted by Portrrey and Berry (1997). They did not find significant race effect with respect to social capital measures such as respondents’ sense of community or levels of participation in neighborhood associations. 53 In the context of police work, however, the relationship between race and social capital might be more straightforward. For example, research shows that minority officers experience more isolation (Buzawa, 1981) and receive less encouragement (Milutinovich, 1977) than their white counterparts. Minority officers may also face “exclusion from informal channels of support and information” (Ellison & Genz, 1983), which may lead to negative consequences in terms of promotions. For example, Carter (1986) found evidence that Hispanic officers in one department believed that the administration discriminated in hiring promotions. Officers in Haarr’s (1997) study also believed that the department made hiring decisions based on race: white officers thought they were biased in favor of minority officers, while rrrinority officers thought they were biased in favor of white officers. Black and Asian officers working in Great Britain identified many ways in which they were omitted from full participation by their co- workers (Holdaway & Barron, 1997). The majority of the evidence (although much of it is dated) suggests that minority officers would have less social capital than their white counterparts. This prediction might be inaccurate, however, in departments that have significant minority representation (and at all ranks), or that have a history of cooperative and supportive relations between officers of different races. While this is not the case in the two departments involved in the current study, minority officers might invest more in relationships in order to counteract their marginalized status. The proposed research could clarify these issues. 54 Education and Social Capital Social capital researchers have long been interested in the link between education and social capital. Not surprisingly, the available evidence indicates that these variables share a positive and mutually enforcing relationship. Social capital is a resource which facilitates successful school outcomes (Bourdieu, 1984; Coleman, 1988; Furstenberg & Hughes, 1995; Teachman, Paasch, & Carver, 1997) and in turn, the size of people’s social networks tends to increase as they become more educated (Antonucci et al., 1998; Edwards & Foley, 1997; Moore, 1990; Stanton-Salazar & Sanford, 1995; Robinson & Morash, 2000). Additionally, F urstenberg and Hughes (1995) found that completion of high school and enrollment in college were related to many social capital measures in a positive direction. Brehm and Rahn (1997) found that education and civic participation shared a strong positive relationship; civic participation is ofien considered a component of social capital (Pumam, 1995). In the context of policing, however, the available evidence suggests that a college education may decrease social capital. Stevenson (1988) found that more educated officers experienced higher levels of burnout and social isolation that their less educated counterparts. It is important to explore the relationship between education and social capital in the unique organizational context of policing. It might be expected that more educated officers do not have higher levels of social capital if they work in organizational environments that neither support nor reward educational achievement. Tenure and Social Capital The relationship between officer tenure and attitudes has been more thoroughly 55 investigated, beginning with Niederhoffer’s (1967) finding that cynicism increases with time on the job. The least cynical officers are those with less than two years of experience (Wilt & Bannon, 1976). In general, research indicates that other negative attitudes also become more predominant with age. For example, as tenure increases so do negative attitudes toward domestic violence victims (Robinson & Chandek, 2000b) and community policing (Lewis, Rosenberg, & Lewis, 1999; cf. Lurigio & Skogan, 1994). Job satisfaction also decreases with tenure (Hoath, Schneider & Starr, 1998). Relevant to social capital, officers with more eXperience tend to hold more negative views about their work relationships, are more cynical about the flow of information between superiors and subordinates (Lewis, Rosenberg, & Sigler, 1999), and perceptions of supervisor support decrease (Winfree & Newbold, 1999). Most of the available evidence suggests a negative relationship between officer tenure and any positive outcome related to their work, and the present research could help us determine whether this is also the case with social capital. WorkIEnvironment and Social Capital There is also research to suggest that social capital might vary according to the work environment in which the work is embedded. For example, in a study designed to assess the ability of the Michigan Victim Assistance Academy (MVAA) to increase the social capital of victim assistance providers, qualitative and quantitative analyses revealed that participants who reported substantial resources in their workplace (e.g., support from supervisors and co-workers, adequate staffing levels) were better able to utilize the social capital gained from attending the MVAA (Robinson & Morash, 2000). Specifically, 56 participants in supportive workplace environments were more likely to expand and improve their networks of relationships relevant to improving assistance to crime victims to a greater extent compared to participants who faced barriers in their workplace (e. g., negative attitudes from supervisors or co-workers, lack of time, money, or staffing, or organizational problems). In short, it appeared that some workplace environments helped rather than hindered the utilization of workers’ social capital. In a recent study examining community policing using a social capital framework, it was also apparent that the organizational context mattered a great deal (Pino, 2001). The implementation of community policing in “Small City” Iowa faced its biggest challenge from the police department itself. In particular, the department was always understaffed and underfunded, creating a situation where patrol officers were forced to work a lot of overtime to achieve adequate patrol levels. This contributed to patrol officers’ animosity and lack of trust toward the few community policing officers who were hired under a federal grant. This lack of trust among police also generated a lack of trust between police and the neighborhood groups with whom they were supposed to create partnerships. Pino (2001) summarized the organizational effect, “in a climate of insufficient resources, an add-on COP program, and a lack of trust among officers, COP was doomed to not live up to its potential” (p. 209). Despite the existence of any social capital among officers, it would appear that the negative organizational climate was in effect a workplace barrier that could not be overcome. Given the possibility that the organization can have an overwhelming influence on not only levels of police social capital, but the utilization of police social capital toward 57 community policing goals, the current study investigates whether the relationship between social capital and community policing is moderated by the department in which the officer works. In addition, officers’ perception of their departments’ support of community policing might also moderate the relationship between police social capital and community policing. It is an important indication of the officer’s work environment and will also be included in the interaction models. 58 CHAPTER 4 DATA AND MEASUREMENT OF VARIABLES The Project on Policing Neighborhoods This study involves secondary data analysis from the Project on Policing Neighborhoods (POPN), a large-scale study of police behavior funded by the National Institute of Justice. Data for the study were collected from the Indianapolis, Indiana and St. Petersburg, Florida Police Departments. This study was conducted during the summers of 1996 and 1997, respectively, and involved two primary sources of data relevant to the current study: Systematic Social Observation (SSO) and structured interviews of police officers. Each method of data collection is described below. Description of Data Collection Systematic Socipl ObservLiop The primary feature of the POPN is the systematic observation of police officers at both research sites. It is ideal to have a comprehensive set of measures when investigating police behavior. Official data, citizen and officer surveys, and observational data are all useful in this regard. Observational data, however, may be particularly useful for assessing officer performance during police-citizen interactions — Opening up for examination the “black box” of police performance (Wycoff, 1982), or the “process” of policing (Mastrofski, 1996, 1999; Reiner, 1998). In short, observational data allow for a more accurate description of the craft of policing. Data are collected first-hand, rather 59 than relying on second hand sources. Fieldwork in Indianapolis and St. Petersburg was conducted during the summer months. Trained observers accompanied patrol officers during their normally scheduled shifts. During ride-alongs, observers took notes on the behavior of patrol officers, as well as other officers (peers and supervisors) and the citizens with whom they interacted. At the conclusion of these observational sessions, observers used their notes to provide detailed narrative accounts of the rides. This information was then converted into coded data using observation instruments designed specifically for the project. The observation instruments consisted of four forms: ride form, activity form, encounter form, citizen form. The observational data therefore contain four levels of analysis. One ride form was completed for every ride-along, and included information on the site, district, rank, and shift of the officer. The activity form was used for events that were not classified as “encounters” with other police or citizens (i.e., these behaviors were typically performed alone). This form included the type of activity in which the officer was engaged, the length of the activity, the type of problem at which the activity was directed, and whether the activity was part of a long-term plan or project. The encounter form was used to code information about situations in which police engaged in some form of verbal or physical contact with a member of the public. Encounters were classified into three categories. Brief encounters involved contact with the public that lasted less than one rrrinute and involved police business, such as an officer telling someone to “move along.” In these encounters fewer than three exchanges (verbal or gestures) between the police and the public occurred. Casual encounters 60 involved contact with the public, but no police business, such as an officer running a personal errand that involved talking with a clerk. Full encounters were police-public contacts that lasted longer than one minute and also involved police business. During these encounters words and/or gestures were exchanged more than three times. Encounters that lasted less than one minute but involved the threat of violence by either police or citizens were also coded as full encounters. The encounter form was used to code information such as the length of the encounter, other participants in the encounter (i.e., officers, citizens, or both), the type of problem at which the encounter was directed, and the type of decisions that were made during the encounter. Lastly, the citizen form captured information such as the age, race, sex, income, and demeanor of all citizens involved in the encounter. Structured Interviews of Officers Structured interviews were conducted with patrol officers, sergeants and lieutenants in both sites by trained interviewers during the officer’s regular work shift. The interviews were designed to capture information on a variety of t0pics, such as the officer’s beliefs about proper police roles, goals, and priorities; the officers’ perceptions of their work group and supervisor; and their attitudes toward community policing. Demographic information (e. g., race, sex) and background characteristics (e.g., education, tenure) were also obtained. Sample The current study uses both data sources for the measurement of independent and dependent variables. Trained observers collected and coded observational data during 61 361 ride-alongs in Indianapolis and 368 ride-alongs in St. Petersburg (totaling 729 rides). Ride-alongs lasted the duration of an officer’s regular shift (8 hours in St. Petersburg and 8.5 hours in Indianapolis), resulting in more than 5,700 hours of field observation (Parks et al., 1999). Some officers were observed during more than one ride-along, some just during one ride-along, and others were not observed at all. A majority of officers in each site participated in the structured interview, resulting in a total of 728 surveyed officers. In Indianapolis, 93% of the 426 patrol officers were interviewed; in St. Petersburg 98% of the department’s 246 patrol officers completed the interview (Paoline, Myers, & Worden, 2000). Observational and survey data were merged at the officer level to obtain a sample of officers that had responses to all measures necessary for a test of the conceptual model proposed in the current study. Dependent measures were derived from the observational data and independent measures were obtained from the officer surveys. The sample of officers who both completed the interview and were observed by the POPN include 176 officers from Indianapolis and 142 officers from St. Petersburg. The total sample to be analyzed in the present study consists of 318 officers. The reason for the reduction in sample size (fi'om 728 to 318 officers) is that while most officers were interviewed, not all officers were observed during ride-alongs. Some officers were observed multiple times, instead of each officer being observed at least once, because the sampling plan was designed according to rides rather than officers. The sampling plan was created to ensure that rides were conducted for (1) every work shift for all beats in both sites, (2) all units working in all beats (3) during days of the week that varied in busyness (Parks et al., 1999). Consequently, the POPN captured 62 multiple observations for some officers, but no observations of others. The 318 officers in this sample were observed a minimum of once and a maximum of 10 rides, with the average officer observed for approximately two rides. Almost half (44%) were observed for one ride, while 25% were observed for two rides, and 31% were observed for three or more rides. The ride-based sampling strategy does have implications for this sample: about half of the officers were observed only once and about half received multiple observations. Whether the amount of observation varies according to characteristics of the officers, their work environment, their social capital, and their community policing performance is discussed in the next section. The sampling strategy developed for the POPN is not ideal for the purposes of the present study. A more suitable sampling plan for this research would be based around officers rather than rides: the focus would be on observing every type of officer rather than every type of ride. An officer-based sampling plan would have increased the sample size of officers suitable for study and also avoided any biases resulting from comparing officers who have been observed for various lengths of time. It should be noted that these biases were addressed by standardizing the observational data to account for officers being observed for different lengths of time (see the next section for a detailed discussion of the measurement of the dependent variables). Furthermore, given the objectives of the present study, it is imperative to conduct the analyses at the officer level because social capital is an attribute of people, not rides. While the decision to conduct the analyses at the officer level might not be methodologically intuitive (given the ride- level sampling strategy), it reflects the theoretical framework of the current study. Measurement of Dependent Variables 63 The present study is concerned with identifying the factors that significantly impact officer performance of community policing. Based on previous research that has identified three categories of activities guided by a community policing philosophy (community engagement, problem-solving, and providing assistance to citizens), the dependent variables are operationalized using six activities that reflect these‘community policing dimensions (more information is provided in Table 2, discussed in the next section). The six measures of community policing include: providing comfort to citizens; providing information to citizens; providing referrals to citizens; attending community meetings; problem-solving activity; and crime prevention activity. Two dependent variables that provide different indicators of officer productivity were created from the six measures: (1) the number of community policing acts performed, per citizen encountered by the officer during the data collection period, and (2) the number of minutes the officer engaged in community policing activities, per 8- hour shift2 worked by the officer during the data collection period. Creating two dependent measures avoided the problem of summing indicators that were measured at different levels of analysis. Specifically, three community policing indicators are measured at the activity level, and three are measured at the encounter level. Consequently, some of the community policing indicators are collected at the level where it makes intuitive sense to count the number of citizens receiving the act, while the other community policing indicators lend themselves to a measurement of the number of 2 St. Petersburg officers worked 8-hour shifts while Indianapolis officers worked 8.5-hour shifts. The decision was made to standardize the time measure by eight hours because most police departments use shifts of this duration. Indianapolis is the exception rather than the rule. 64 minutes the officer was engaged in the activity. Community policing acts (providing comfort, information, and referrals to citizens) were coded from the observational data gathered during the ride-alongs. The citizen form was used to quantify information about police-citizen encounters. This allowed for a count of how many citizens were provided comfort, information, and/or referrals from each officer during the observational period. The ‘comfort’ indicator was derived from the question, “During the encounter, did the police comfort or reassure. the citizen?” This was a yes/no question where comfort was only counted when it was preformed by the primary officer under observation, or the primary officer along with his or her partner or other police at the scene. Provided below are excerpts from the narratives that provide examples of how police provide comfort to citizens. The primary officer under observation is designated 01 (these are the officers included in my sample), while his or her partner is designated 02 and other police at the scene are designated 03, 04, etc. Citizens involved in the encounter are designated C1, C2, C3, etc. > At a park where the marchers are dispersing, 02 is lecturing the children on their bad behavior during the march. 02 punishes the children by saying that they will not be taken for a treat after the march. The children are very upset and one in particular appears to be crying. 01 walks up to C1, who is a black male about 10 years old, and lower class based on dirty clothing. 01 comforts C1 and tells him to get in the van to get a ride home. C1 is very upset and his head hangs low. Cl acknowledges Ol's request and heads into the van. 01 leaves the scene. v 01 started walking to her patrol car when she spotted a black female walking up to the emergency room doors. Ol asked the lady if she is the mother of the accident victim fi'om encounter 21. Cl, black female about 40 years of age, stated that she is the mother. C1 is middle class based on her attire. She is wearing pants and a short sleeved shirt. C1 is neat in appearance. Ol explained to Cl what occurred and she explained that her son was taken to another hospital 65 for treatment. C1 seemed very concerned for her son’s well being. 01 reassured her that he was going to be all right. The communication between the two was very friendly. 01 said that she could follow her over to the hospital and C1 stated that she knows where the hospital is and that she will meet her over there. The measure ‘providing inforrnation’ was taken from the question, “Did the police provide this citizen information on how to deal with a problem on their own initiative (without the citizen’s request)?” This was a yes/no question. The following examples demonstrate police providing information in practice: v 01 walked to the front door of the house which was located in a residential neighborhood. The door was opened by C1, a lS-year-old black female. 01 asked C1 if she had called the police and C1 stated that her mother had called the police. C1 asked 01 to come inside and said she would go get her mother. Before C1 left the room 01 asked her if the dog sitting in the living room would bite. C1 told 01 that her dog wouldn’t bite and 01 began to pet the dog hesitantly. C1 returned with her mother C2, a black female approximately 34 years of age. C2 explained that she had a restraining order against her husband and that he had been at her window harassing her. C2 stated that he left when she said she was going to call the police. C2 also told 01 that there is a warrant for her husband’s arrest. At this point 01 asked C2 for a description of her husband and C2 told 01 his name and gave a brief physical description of him. C1 told 01 where her step-father usually stays and the type of car he drives. C2 left the room to get her husband’s social security number... At this point C2 returned with her husband’s social security number. 01 told C2 that he was going to do a report and would keep his eye out for him during the night. 01 told C2 that if he returned she should call the police. 01 told C2 to call 911 and then leave the phone off the hook. 01 told C2 that they would get an emergency 911 run and would get to her a lot faster if she did this. C1 and C2 thanked 01 and 01 wished them a nice night. > C1 (a black female, 41 years old, middle class based on neat appearance and driving'a newer model Toyota passenger car; upset but respectful) drove up and told 01 that she was the complainant. She said that her husband had called her from a store down the road and that he was out of breath and sounded really worried that someone was after him, and she was very worried about his welfare. 03 then departed to the store. She told 01 that her husband was out of breath when he called because he had run from the car wash to the store to escape the 66 robber. She said she did not know why her husband insisted on washing his car that late at night. [When] 03 returned to the car [he] was with C2 (a black male, 45 years old, middle class based on dress and driving a newer pickup; upset but respectfirl), who was Cl's husband. C2 said he had just started to wash his truck when a black male wearing a black cap and green shirt stepped out of some bushes, pulled out a handgun and started to cock it. When C2 saw that, he sprayed the black male with a car wash hose and then took off running and called his wife. He said he probably could not identify the man again if he saw him. 01 advised C2 that there had been many robberies in this area and that C2 should wait until daylight to wash his vehicle. 01 provided C2 a pamphlet on victims’ rights, and C1 and C2 then thanked 01 and left the area. 02 and 03 also departed. The ‘referral’ indicator was supplied by the question, “Did the police ask/tell the citizen to seek the help of other service agencies to solve the problem?” The police could suggest, request, try persuasion, try negotiation, or command the citizen to seek the help of an agency. Examples of police providing citizens with referrals are provided below. > As 01 was patrolling a residential neighborhood a black male waved at the officer and asked him to stop. C1, a black male of approximately 45 years of age was standing at the side of the road and walked over to the patrol car. C1 told 01 that he wanted some advice on a problem he was having. C1 told 01 that he had his car painted several weeks ago and that he was not pleased with the job the person had done. He said that when he returned the car to the individual they refused to fix the paint job. At this point 01 explained that this was a civil matter and that he might have a case in civil court. 01 gave C1 a brochure about small claims court and asked C1 some questions. 01 asked Cl if he had a receipt and C1 said that he didn’t have a receipt. C1 said that he had several witnesses though. 01 told C1 that he might not have a very strong case if he didn’t have a receipt or contract for the work but that the cost of the small claims court was only $40. C1 thanked 01 for his advice and the encounter ended. The encounter was not wimessed by any bystanders. Both C1 and 01 interacted in a business- like manner. > C1, a middle class white female in her late 205, came to the door of the given address. The house was located in a small neighborhood with very nice houses and yards. C1 was dressed in clean shorts and a tank top. She had two small children with her, a boy aged 5 and a girl aged 2. She told 01 that her concern 67 was with children getting hurt in the dump site. 01 shook his head and said he could understand that. Cl said her husband was outside videotaping the area. We followed Cl through her yard then walked through some very tall weeds. As we got through the weeds, it began to smell very strongly of cow manure. C1 told 01 that trucks had been dumping all these materials in this vacant lot and there were some questionable materials in it. She said every time it rained the contaminated water ran into their yard and probably down to the septic tank. She said it was probably getting into the water. When we got past the weeds, there were several large hills of dirt and broken up concrete. C2, a middle class white male in his late 205, was standing on one of the hills videotaping a large hole filled with very dirty water and building material. C2 came down and asked 01 if there was anything they could do to stop the company from dumping. C1 said she had already called the Health Department. 01 said that they wouldn’t listen to him any more than they listened to her. He recommended that she call the Zoning Department, building inspectors, and/or the Environmental Protection Agency. 01 said that the EPA might test the water if they (C1 and C2) took a sample of it to them. 01 also suggested that they call the local news station. C2 said that they had called one of them and they were supposed to be coming out soon to do a story on it. 01 told them to keep pursuing the news station because once they made a big story out of it, the agencies involved would have to respond... 01 suggested that C1, C2, and C3 send fliers to their neighbors and inform them of the conditions nearby. He said that if the neighborhood banded together, they would probably get better results. He also said that if nothing else worked, they could file a class action lawsuit... He told me [observer] this situation was a good example of community policing: giving advice to citizens about resources in the community that they can go to when there really isn’t anything the police can do. The second dependent variable was derived from activity-level data. Activities are distinguished from encounters in that the former do not necessarily involve an interaction with a citizen, while the latter do. Narrative information was quantified and allowed for identification of activities where officers engaged in problem-solving, crime prevention, or attending community meetings. The variable for ‘community meetings’ was obtained ftom the question, “Did this activity involve a meeting with representatives of a citizen organization?” Citizen organizations could include neighborhood or other area-based groups, victim advocate groups, business groups, church or religious groups, school groups or other unspecified community groups. Additionally, the coding 68 instructions required that representatives of the organization had to be acting as members on behalf of that organization for the activity to count as a community meeting. Examples of officers spending time at community meetings are provided below: > En route to a meeting about weekly park activities at the Leisure Services department. When it came time for the meeting, the head of the department directed everyone to the conference room. 01 sat through the meeting which was a weekly thing to discuss the activities at a local park every Sunday. Those attending were some members of Vista, a police supervisor in the department, another CPO in charge of some of the park, a representative for activities in the park and another city representative. The meeting was supposed to be to discuss how the activities in the park went the past Sunday. Vista members complained about how their “Father’s day in the park” activity there the past Sunday had been cut shorter than they would have liked due to a lack of funding. One Vista member complained about the marijuana smoking that “was allowed” to go on in the park and wanted to lorow why this and public drinking were not being punished. The police supervisor from the department, a black male approximately 50 years old, claimed that those activities were illegal and were not being allowed to go on by the police who worked the park. Others issues were brought up during the meeting as well. 01 voiced some citizens’ concerns over public urination and the need to have some portable toilets placed in the park. > The meeting was already in progress. Everyone attending the meeting was seated around a long wooden table. 01 took a seat on a couch located near one end of the table. There were 16 people attending this meeting. The meeting was held by the K Business Association. It consisted of business owners located on K Street. They were discussing a proposal for the 1st Annual K Baseball Festival (to celebrate the new baseball team). They were also discussing plans to convert a large portion of K Street into a commercial district. The goal was to offset the negative image given to K Street by the riots. Toward the end of the meeting, the group addressed 01 and asked if she had any suggestions based on their goals. 01 stated that the most important thing is that if a problem arises, don’t wait for the problem to get out of hand before calling the police. Contact the police during the initial stages of the project. 01 made her services available to everyone in the group and she handed out a few business cards to the group. The meeting officially concluded. The ‘problem-solving’ indicator was derived from the question, “Was this activity 69 part of a long-term plan or project to deal with a problem?” Long-term was defined as longer than the ride being observed. Furthermore, the officer must have planned this activity prior to the ride. Plans that were deve10ped spontaneously or during the ride were not considered long-term. Plans could focus on specific people or locations, this kind of problem or crime in general, or unspecified long-term plans. Below are examples of problem-solving activities from the narratives: D 01 arrives at the scene where the drug march will begin. It is primarily a low- class neighborhood. Many of the houses are boarded-up. The residences are very run-down, with garbage in the yards. Windows do not have blinds or drapes, but instead are covered with old dirty blankets. 01 is told by 02 to follow the back of the march in the patrol car. Many of the citizens are wearing yellow T-shirts that say “up with hope, down with dope,” the main chant of the march. The citizens walk down the street, some with megaphones, shouting this slogan as well as several others. Residents of the neighborhood come out of their homes to see what is going on. Some are pleased with the march, but others shout profanities at the marchers, telling them to leave. The marchers stop at one place [supposedly a known drug selling spot] and continue their chanting. They chant over and over differing slogans, while the neighborhood citizens stand in their front yards and watch. 01 gets out of her car and stands with the marchers sometimes clapping and shouting the slogans. C3 described how the Center attracts vagrants who congregate, beg, sleep, drink, and urinate about his property and other businesses along this street. C3 explained he is losing customers. The pe0ple who have done business with him in the past complain about the vagrants bothering them. He has spoken to some of the other business owners on this street. He intends to write a letter to the legislator who would be responsible for this district. Further, C3 would like police to help him. 01 told the owner that he had been working on the problem for the past year. He mentioned other business owners who had contacted him about the same issues. 01 asked C3 to inform him of any action C3 may take in organizing the business owners in the area. 01 added he would like a copy of the letter C3 plans to send to the legislator. Ol informed C3 of the legislator responsible for this district. Further, 01 gave C3 the name and address of the president of the neighborhood association who may also be able to offer assistance to C3. 01 told C3 that signs that read “no trespassing” needed to be posted on the property. [Later in the same shift...] The business owner with whom 01 spoke earlier had left the store to have lunch. His business partner and wife talked to 01. Ol helped her complete the [trespassing] forms. 01 stated she and 70 her husband needed to post “no trespassing” signs in several places about the property. During the encounter 01 was helpful and fiiendly. The business partner’s wife calmly filled out the necessary paperwork. She seemed pleased that police would take some action if they complained about the vagrants. The final community policing activity, ‘crime prevention’ was derived from the following question, “During this activity, were the police trying to prevent the occurrence or recurrence of the problem?” This was a yes/no question where the activity was only coded as crime prevention if the officer’s efforts were focused on a period beyond the end of the shift. In other words, the action taken by the officer must be clearly future oriented. v 01 stopped in a large parking lot in fiont of the store (a home maintenance outlet). He said that the store had suffered a string of shoplifting and other problems, but that since he had started parking there regularly in visible areas, they had only one small shoplifting reported. 01 said that his theory was that just by being visible to the public, the police can stop a lot of illegal activity because people see they are being watched and are likely to be more careful. v 01 and 02 go to the ground level of the police station. They discussed getting new dead bolts with keys. Ol explained that they were currently using an apartment (located in a degraded, lower class apartment complex) as an office and wanted to get new locks for the door. They were concerned that people would attempt to break into the apartment once they found out that the police were using it as a pseudo headquarters. 01 continued by stating that the local drug dealers had already thrown rocks through the apartrnent’s windows after news of their presence got around. 01 and 02 met with a black male in his 305, a maintenance worker employed by the police department. 01 informed the maintenance worker about getting the locks installed. 01 gave the worker the address of the apartment. The worker said he would relay the request to his superiors. 01 gave the worker her pager number and asked that his supervisor get in contact with her as soon as possible. 71 These narrative examples provide an indication of what types of police activities are conceptualized as community policing in the current study. The specific construction of the two measures of community policing (time per 8-hour shift and acts per citizen) is discussed in the next section, followed by a presentation of the descriptive statistics for these variables and the six community policing indicators used to create them. Providing Acts of Communig Policing to Citizens This dependent variable (CP Acts) represents the number of citizens receiving community policing acts by the officer, divided by the total number of citizens coming into contact with the officer during the data collection period. Providing community policing to citizens is measured by the number of times the officer (1) provided comfort or reassurance to citizens, (2) provided referrals to citizens, and (3) provided information to citizens. These three measures were summed to provide a variable representing the total number of community policing acts the officer performed during the data collection period. According to Carrnines and Zeller (1979), scales that produce reliability coefficients greater than .70 are considered reliable. The reliability coefficient (Cronbach’s Alpha=.60) indicates that these three activities fall below the conventional standard; therefore findings related to this variable should be interpreted with caution. Due to the method of data collection (i.e., field observation where some officers were observed more than others), the total number of community policing acts was divided by the total number of citizens with whom the officer came into contact during the data collection period. The resulting variable (CP Acts) is therefore a standardized measure of the number of community policing acts provided by officers per citizen encountered during the data collection period. Despite this standardization process, 72 results could still be impacted by the ride-based sampling strategy and therefore additional tests were performed on this variable. Analyses were conducted to determine whether CP Acts varied significantly depending on the number of rides for which the officer was observed. Results indicated that the mean CP Acts did vary according to amount of observation: officers observed for one ride provided about one CP Act; officers observed twice provided about two CP Acts; officers observed three or more times provided almost 5 CP Acts F (2, N = 318) = 94.22, p <.001. Despite the standardization of this variable, officers with multiple observations tended to provide more CP Acts than officers observed only once. What is different about officers who were observed multiple times? They were compared according to their demographic characteristics, features of their work environment, and their levels of social capital.3 Only three (out of 14) of the independent variables varied significantly according to the number of rides observed. Officers with more education were more likely to be observed more than once F (2, N = 318) = 5.04, p <.01, as were officers working in SPPD x2 (2, N = 318) = 8.54, p <.05. Conversely, officers with higher levels of community policing training were more likely to be observed only once F (2, N = 318) = 3.47, p<.05. These differences should be kept in mind as they indirectly affect the average number of CP Acts provided to citizens. Time Spent Engaged in Community Policing This dependent variable (CP Time) represents the number of minutes an officer spent on community policing activities, per 8-hour shift worked by the officer during the 3 Detailed descriptions of these independent variables are provided in the next section. 73 data collection period. Community policing is measured by the time spent by the officer: (1) engaged in problem-solving activities, (2) engaged in crime prevention activities, and (3) attending community meetings. These three measures were summed to provide a variable representing the amount of time (in minutes) that the officer was engaged in community policing during the data collection period. The reliability coefficient (Alpha=.72) indicates that these three activities exceed the conventional standard of .70; thus, this scale can be considered reliable. Due to the method of data collection (i.e., field observation where some officers were observed more than others), each officer’s community policing minutes were divided by 480 minutes to standardize the measure for an 8-hour shift. The resulting variable (CP Time) is therefore a standardized measure of the number of minutes per shift the officer spent on community policing during the data collection period. Analyses were conducted to determine whether CP Time varied significantly depending on the number of rides for which the officer was observed. Results indicated that mean CP Time did not vary according to the number of observations F (2, N = 318) =.64, p =.53. Overall, this dependent variable appears to have less measurement error and sampling bias compared to CP Acts. Description of Dependent Variables Descriptive statistics for the two dependent variables, and the variables used to create them, are presented in Table 2. 74 Table 2 Measurement and Descriptive Statistics for Dependent Variables. Variable Description m Mp; Mean §._D_, # Citizens receiving comfort/reassurance 0.00 28.00 2.92 3.76 # Citizens receiving information 0.00 45.00 6.19 7.45 # Citizens receiving referrals 0.00 98.00 24.50 20.95 Total # citizens receiving CP acts 0.00 145.00 33.61 29.19 Total # of citizens encountered by officer. 1.00 54.00 17.17 9.25 CP Acts = TOTAL CP ACTS / TOTAL 0.00 14.50 2.42 2.41 CITIZENS ENCOUNTERED “Acts of comforting, providing information, and/or providing referrals per citizen encountered.” Alpha=.60 # Minutes attending community meetings 0.00 319.00 4.46 30.20 # Minutes problem-solving 0.00 640.00 9.37 53.53 # Minutes engaged in crime prevention 0.00 791.00 26.52 87.46 Total # Community Policing Minutes 0.00 1559.00 40.45 148.34 Total Shifts Observed (Total Minutes 0.15 10.13 2.30 1.84 Observed / 480 Minutes) CP Time = TOTAL CP TIME / TOTAL SHIFTS 0.00 194.17 11.02 25.37 N=318 “Minutes spent attending corrrrnunity meetings, problem-solving, or engaged in crime prevention per 8-hour shift.” Alpha=.72 Notes: Values provided for CP Acts reflect the change of one outlier from 34.0 to 14.5. CP Acts and CP Time were transformed to integers for Negative Binomial regression requirements. 75 The three community policing activities comprising the CP Acts variable include comforting or reassuring citizens, giving information to citizens, and/or providing citizens with referrals. During the approximately 3-month long data collection period, the average officer comforted approximately three citizens, provided information to six citizens, and gave referrals to 24 citizens. Summing these three acts reveals that the average officer provided a form of community policing to about 33 citizens during the data collection period. The average officer came into contact with about 17 citizens during the data collection period. Descriptive statistics for the CP Acts variable indicate that the average officer provided more than two acts of community policing per citizen encountered. The variable ranges from a minimum of zero acts per citizen encountered, to a maximum of 15 acts per citizen encountered. The majority of officers (n=290; 91%) provided at least one community policing act per citizen. Similar proportions of officers provided one act per citizen (n=132; 42%) and from 2-5 acts per citizen (n=126; 40%). A small number of officers provided (n=25; 8%) 6-10 acts, and six officers (2%) provided 11-15 acts of community policing per citizen encountered. Only 28 officers (9%) did not provide a single act of community policing during the data collection period. The three community policing activities comprising the CP Time variable include attending community meetings, problem-solving, and/or crime prevention. Each of these indicators is measured in minutes. During the data collection period, the average officer spent about four minutes attending community meetings, about nine rrrinutes problem- solving, and about 26 minutes engaged in crime prevention. Summing these community policing indicators reveals that the average officer spent about 40 minutes on these 76 community policing activities during the data collection period. The average officer was observed for approximately two 8-hours shifts. Descriptive statistics for the CP Acts variable indicate that the average officer spent about 11 minutes per shift on community policing, or roughly 2% of each shift. This variable ranges fiom a minimum of zero minutes per shift, to a maximum of 194 minutes per shift. Unlike the CP Acts variable, the majority of officers (n=200; 63%) spent no time engaged in community policing as measured by problem-solving, attending community meetings, or engaging in crime prevention activities. Per shift observed during the data collection period, 48 officers (15%) spent 1-15 minutes on community policing, 29 officers (9%) spent 16-29 minutes, 18 officers (6%) spent 30-45 minutes, seven officers (2%) spent 46-60 minutes, 11 officers (4%) spent 61-120 minutes, and four officers (1%) spent more than 120 minutes. Removing the officers who spent more than 120 minutes from subsequent bivariate and multivariate analyses (presented in Chapter 6) did not affect the results. Measurement of Independent Variables The independent variables included in the present study are grouped into three categories: (1) officer characteristics, (2) characteristics of the officer’s work environment, and (3) social capital dimensions. The respective measurement of and descriptive statistics for these categories of variables are presented in Tables 3, 4, and 5. Measurement of the variables comprising each category is first described, followed by a discussion of the descriptive statistics for all the independent variables included in this study. 77 Officer Chagrcteristics Officer characteristics include dummy variables for officer race (0=white, 1=Afiican American, Hispanic, Asian or Other) and sex (0=male, l=female). The variable for officer education has eight categories: 1=less than High School, 2=High School/GED diploma, 3=Junior College, 4=Associate’s Degree, 5=two or more years of college, 6=Bachelor’s Degree, 7=some graduate work, 8=Graduate Degree. Officer tenure is an interval-level variable representing the number of years the officer has worked at the department, created by subtracting the year the officer began working for the department from 1996 (Indianapolis officers) and 1997 (St. Petersburg officers). A community policing training scale was constructed from seven types of training that the officer may have received: public speaking; computer/automated systems; community policing principles; code enforcement/civil regulations; mediation; analyzing neighborhood crime data; and organizing community groups. Items were coded so that high values represented more training (l=none, 2=less than one day, 3=1- 2 days, 4=3-5 days, and 5=more than five days). The scale ranged from 7 to 32 and the reliability coefficient for this scale is .81. 78 lz Table 3 Measurement and Descriptive Statistics for Officer Characteristics. Variable Description Values % / Mean SD. Female Officer is female. 1 = yes 20.8 0.41 0 = no 79.2 Non-white Officer is non-white. 1 = yes 28.6 0.45 0 = no 71.4 Education Officer’s highest level 1 = Less than H.S. 0.7 1.63 of education. 2 = HS/GED 16.4 3 = Jr. College 26.6 4 = Assoc. Degree 6.8 5 = 2+ yrs. College 17.4 6 = Bach. Degree 27.3 7 = Some Grad. 4.4 8 = Grad. Degree 0.3 Tenure Years at department. 0-31 9. l 7.21 CP Training Scale of 7 types of Community 7-32 15.6 5.25 Policing training (Alpha = .81). Public Speaking 1 = None 62.2 1.29 2 = < 1 day 15.6 3 = 1 to 2 days 9.5 4 = 3 to 5 days 3.4 5 = > 5 days 9.2 Computer/Automated 1 = None 2.4 0.98 Information Systems 2 = < 1 day 21.1 3= 1 t02days 41.5 4 = 3 to 5 days 23.8 5 = > 5 days 11.2 79 Table 3 (cont’d). Concepts/Principles of CP Code Enforcement/Civil Regulations Mediation Using Crime Data to Solve Problems Organizing Community Groups 1=None 2=<1¢w 3=1t02days 4=3t05days 5=>5days 1=None 2=<1day 3=1t02days 4=3t05days 5=>5days 1=None 2=<1mw 3=1t02days 4=3t05days 5=>5days 1=None 2=<1day 3=1t02days 4=3t05days 5=>5days 1=None 2=<1day 3=1t02days 4=3t05days 5=>5days 9.6 16.1 34.6 26.7 13.0 33.0 28.9 19.9 8.2 10.0 54.8 18.5 16.4 6.5 3.8 50.2 29.4 13.7 4.8 2.0 76.4 13.4 6.8 1.4 2.1 1.28. 1.14 0.99 0.84 N=318 80 Work Environment The second group of independent variables, officer’s work environment, consists of five items. Three items are dummy variables: the officer works during a day shift (1=yes, 0=other shift), the officer has a community policing assignment (0=general patrol assignment, 1=community policing assignment) and the department where the officer works (1=Indianapolis, 0=St. Petersburg). The next item in this category of variables is an additive scale containing seven issues the officer perceives to be a major problem in his or her beat: theft or burglary; litter and trash; vandalism of cars and property; drug dealing; gangs; loitering; and abandoned buildings. All items in this scale were coded as follows: 1=not a problem, 2=minor problem, 3=major problem. The scale ranged from 7 to 21 and the reliability coefficient for the scale is .72. The fifth item is a scale that was created to reflect the officer’s perception of whether his or her department is supportive of community policing. This scale includes five items that were coded so that high values represent the officer’s belief that the department does support community policing (l=poor, 2=fair, 3=good, 4=excellent). Statements comprising this scale include “the department clarifies the role of officers in community policing,” “the department fairly distributes the workload of community policing and patrol officers,” “the department gives officers enough time for community policing,” “the department gives officers information for community policing,” and “the department rewards officers for community policing.” The scale ranged from 5 to 20 and the reliability coefficient (Cronbach’s alpha) for this scale is .82. 81 Table 4 Measurement and Descriptive Statistics for Work Environment Variables. Variable Description Values % / Mean S_.IL Department Officer’s department 1 = Indianapolis 59.7 0.49 0 = St. Petersburg 40.3 Day Shift Officer works day shift 1 = yes 37.7 0.49 0 = no 62.3 CPA Community Policing Assignment 1 = yes 37.4 0.48 0 = no 62.6 Beat Problems Officers’ perceptions of 7 beat 7-21 15.7 2.96 Scale problems (Alpha = .72) Theft 1 = Not a problem 1 4 0.53 2 = Minor problem 50.3 3 = Major problem 48.3 Litter 1 = Not a problem 20.0 0.73 2 = Minor problem 44.8 3 = Major problem 35.2 Vandalism 1 = Not a problem 15.9 0.64 2 = Minor problem 57.8 3 = Major problem 26.3 Drug Dealing 1 = Not a problem 7.3 0.63 2 = Minor problem 28.8 3 = Major problem 63.9 Gangs 1 = Not a problem 26.3 0.77 2 = Minor problem 40.1 3 = Major problem 33.6 82 Table 4 (cont’d). Loitering 1 = Not a problem 17.2 0.74 2 = Minor problem 37.6 3 = Major problem 45.2 Abandoned Buildings 1 = Not a problem 25.6 0.78 2 = Minor problem 38.8 3 = Major problem 35.6 Dept. Pro CP Officer’s perceptions of dept. 5-20 9.9 3.34 Scale support of CP (Alpha = .82). Dept. clarifies role of officers in 1 = poor 29.5 0.90 community policing. 2 = fair 34.2 3 = good 30.8 4 = excellent 5.5 Dept. fairly distributes workload 1 = poor 46.0 0.91 of community policing and patrol 2 = fair 30.4 officers. 3 = good 18.3 4 = excellent 5.2 Dept. gives officers enough time 1 = poor 46.4 0.94 for community policing. 2 = fair 28.2 3 = good 19.2 4 = excellent 6.2 Dept. gives officers info for 1 = poor 22.7 0.90 community policing. 2 = fair 40.5 3 = good 28.2 4 = excellent 8.6 Dept. rewards officers for 1 = poor 33.8 0.78 community policing. 2 = fair 45.6 3 = good 18.1 4 = excellent 2.4 N=318 83 Social Capital Dimepsions Social capital dimensions include: (1) level of trust, (2) cooperative exchanges, (3) group cohesion, and (4) social support. Table 5 contains the measurement and descriptive statistics for each of the four social capital dimensions. Factor analyses were conducted on three of the four social capital dimensions (cooperative exchanges, group cohesion, social support), as they include multiple indicators. Results of those analyses are presented in Table 6, located at the end of this section. Table 5 Measurement and Descriptive Statistics for Social Capital Dimensions. Variable Description Trust Officer has complete faith in supervisor. Cooperation Officer gathers public safety info from other officers. Officer gathers public safety info from supervisor. Proportion of unit that officer would share hard-to-get info. Values 1 = disagree strongly 2 = disagree somewhat 3 = agree somewhat 4 = agree strongly 1 = never 2 = rarely 3 = sometimes 4 = often 1 = never 2 = rarely 3 = sometimes 4 = often 1 = none 2=afiw 3 = about half 4 = all or most 5.2 11.9 29.0 53.8 0.0 6.2 33.1 60.7 5.9 26.2 45.9 22.1 0.3 15.4 10.9 73.4 MEL 0.88 0.61 0.83 0.76 Table 5 (cont’d). Group Cohesion Officer rating of work unit. 1 = not as good 3.1 0.55 2 = about the same 36.3 3 = better than most 60.6 Proportion of unit that officer 1 = none 2.1 0.87 considers to be friends. 2 = a few 19.9 3 = about half 21.0 4 = all or most 57.0 Officer enjoys working with 1 = disagree strongly 5.2 0.87 supervisor. 2 = disagree somewhat 9.3 3 = agree somewhat 11.4 4 = agree strongly 74.0 Support Supervisor supports officer 1 = disagree strongly 5.3 0.85 when he/she is right. , 2 = disagree somewhat 8.5 3 = agree somewhat 28.9 4 = agree strongly 57.4 Supervisor seldom criticizes 1 = disagree strongly 3.8 0.77 officer. 2 = disagree somewhat 5.9 3 = agree somewhat 19.4 4 = agree strongly 70.8 Supervisor looks out for 1 = disagree strongly 4.5 0.82 welfare of subordinates. 2 = disagree somewhat 7.3 3 = agree somewhat 21.6 4 = agree strongly 66.6 N=3 1 8 85 As T able 5 indicates, level 0 f II'ust is measured by the officer’s response to the statement “I have complete faith in my Supervisor,“ and is coded -1=disagr€6 strongly, 2=<1isagree somewhat, 3=agree somewhat, and 4=agree strongly. High values therefore represent a higher degree of trust Steffi-reported by the Officer about his or her supervisor. The COOperative exchanges dimension is measured by three items. One item measures the frequency that the officer gathers pubh'c Safety information from other officers, and one item measures the frequency that the officer gathers public safety information from his or her supervisor. Both items are coded as 1=never 2=rar61Y’ 3=sometimes, and 4=often. The third item is the officer’s response t 0 the question, ‘ . . - . t you obtained some hard-to— get information about the Identity of an off ndet causing a X0 6 Qt trouble 'm your district, With how many of the officers in Yew Uni ould you sham t w read?” 3‘10“?” 311d COded as 1=n0ne, 2=feW, 3=abOUt half {If—all moSt : ~ or ' this inform ' ‘ lded com onent, o with an Componentsfac‘OY Analers yle one P r factor, fro these it6 6 Eigenvalue of \. 3% . The Kaiser-Gunman rule suggests that factors With an E i gent/81113 greater than one should be retained (Kim & Mueller, 1978)- The factor expj ai 35 almost 40% of the variance in the included variables. Cronbach’s Alpha (.23) indicates that this tor falls well below the conventional standard of reliability (.70). Overall’ there 1.5 fac substantial measurement error in this factor and results should be interpreted With caution. Group COhesion is measured by three items. F irst, Officers were asked to give a —— 4 Webster‘s dictionary defines faith as “confidence or trust in a pf? variable as a measure of trust, although it could also be measurlng rson 01' thing.” I am the re ing ‘ confidence or faith fore us " this 86 t' of their w ' ' ta mg ork unit. Their responses were coded as f0110ws: l‘not as good asmost others, 2=abou tthe same as most others, and 3=better than most other units. The second item represents a proportion of officers in the reSpondent’s k unit that he or she wor considers friends, coded as 1=none, 2=a few 3‘abo thalf d 4 11 or most The thifd a m u , an :3 ‘ item was the officer’s response to the state ment “M . - f son I ’ Y SUpervrsor IS the type 0 per enjoy working with ”5 and was coded 1 5disa , gree Stron _. ' so what gly, 2—drsagree me , 3=agree somewhat, and 4=agree strongly. Principal Components Factor Analysis yielde 30/0 Of one component from these items with an Eigenvalue o f 1 30 The fa t plains 4 - - c or ex t the variance in the included variables. The Alpha (.29) indicates th t thi f ctol’ (103‘5 no a S 3 n the meet the conventional level of reliability and this may have a ental imp results. Three items are used to measure the third social Capital . 51$ support. dlménSiOns 50 6 They represent the Officer" s responses to the statements “M ort m ’ y Sllpe ' 1 5‘39? mi 501' W11 when I am right, ev en if it makes things difficult for him/her,» “Th of e decisions judgments I make are seldom criticized or modified by my SupewiSor ,, and “NY supervisor looks out for the personal welfare of his/her SUbOl'dinates-” A11 items were coded 1=disagree SlToneg, 2=disagTCe somewhat, 3=agree somewhat, and 4§egree Stroneg, Principal Components Factor Analysis yielded one component from these items with an Eigenvalue of1,91, The factor explains 64%. ofthe variance in the included variables. The Alpha (.71) indicates that this factor meets the ConVentional level of reliability and is therefore a reliable measure 0f the scam SUPPOIT Construct ‘— s This item originally read “My supervisor is NOT the type of person I enjoy Working With ., The item w - 'as \QN use oooed. 87 Table 6 Princi alCom onent Factor An al 565 of So - cral Ca ital Dimensions Factor Item _ Factor 0.312 0.558 Cooperation Officer gathers public Safet . from other officers. y lnformation Officer gathers public Safe t . fi'om supervisor. y Information 0'3 83 0'619 Proportion of unit that offic er we share hard—to-get information wi tlil 1d 0488 0698 Alpha .................................................................................................................. OE lgenvalue 1 .fi: ................ Group 0 fficer rating of work unit. 0 w Cohesion 0 ‘572 0. Proportion of unit that officer considers t of] 83 be friends, 0 0.62X Officer enj oys Working with supervison 0.328 ................................................... - - 010$ Alpha 29 - - ---. glgenvalue 1.30 5 /° Variance Explained ‘ 43 .3 3 ”FPO” .SuperVisor supports officer When he/she x J 18 right. 0'79 1 0.89 SUpervisor seldom criticizes Officer. 0 349 0.591 Supervisor looks out for welfare of s b d' O. 77 u or mates. 0-8 7 7 Alpha- ~ “.7, i .............................................................................. Eigenvalue 1,9 1 iiiiiiiiiii % Variance Expw 6 N's?» 1 8 i 5 H 88 Description oflndependent Variables The majority of officers are male (79%) and white (71%), More than one in {our officers holds a bachelor’s degree (2 7%) , and more than half of the officers (56%) have an associate’s degree or more education- The average officer worked in the police depmem for nine years The mean re Sponse for the community policing training scale was 15 -6- Most officers reported receiving no training on organizing community groups (76%), public Speaking (62%), or mediation (55% ). Most officers received some training in computer/automated information systems (98%), Co I] c ePtS/Principles of community policing (90%), or code enforcement/civil regulations (67%)- About half of Officers received some training on using crime data to solve Problems (50%) . Thirty-eight percent of officers worked the day shjfi, 60% Worked mme lmhanapolis police department during the data collection Period, and about one in three {colours officers was a community policing specialist. The mean reSDOnse 1: th beat ‘3 Q e scale was l5 .1 ,indicat‘mg that most officers perceived seVeral issues t be problems in 0 their beats. The problem most frequently described by °ffi°ers as a “major problem” was drug dealing (64%), followed by thefi (48%)? and loitering (450“) The issues most frequently described by officers as “not a problem” were gangs and aband‘m'ed buildinos D (26% each), followed by litter (20%) The Department Support of COmmmli 1y Policing S cale had a mean response of 99, Less than one in ten offi c ers described their department as “excellent” on any of the five items comPfiS ing this scale. Officers most frequently described their department as “poor” on two 0f the items: (1) “my department fairly distributes the workload of community policing and patrol officers” (46%): and (2) “we; gevmment gives officers enough time for corfll’nullity pOIiCing” (460/0), 89 Regarding the social capital dimensions, most officers scored high on level of trust (83% agreed that they had “complete faith” in their supervisors). Items in the cooperation dimension reveal that 61 % often gathered public safety information from other officers, and 730/0 said they wou1d Share hard-to-get information with all or most of the officers in their work group. Fewer cooperative exchanges occurred with supervisors: less than one in four (22%) Often gathered public safety information from their supervisors. The group cohesion dimension {eve a] s that about 6 out of 10 officers consider their work units “better than most others,” (6 1 %) and consider all of most officers in their unit to be friends (57%). A majority o f officers (78° /0) enjoy working with their supervisor. The support dimension also shows a high degree of positiVe sentiment among the Officers, More than eight Of ever-y ten melt officer d max 8 agree supervisor rarely criticizes them (900/0), looks out for the w 61 f {oifia‘es are 0 1‘ their Subo (88%), and supports the 0 fficer when he or she is right, even i f _ ’ngs ay m difficult (86%). 9O CHAPTER 5 ANALYTIC MODELS AND METHODS Analytic Models Regression analyses were conducted on additive models (one per dependent variable) to determine the direct effects of the independent variables on community policing, and then a series of interaction models (seven per dependent variable) to determine whether- the relationship between social Capital and Comfmmity policiflg is moderated by various officer characteristics and features 0 f their Work environment. For the additive models, 14 independent variables were inc 111de in the - i . allys‘s 0 regr‘e's’s‘mn w 6 resultin in roxirnatel l8 cases er v - no 262 officers, g app Y P anable. For the int 31730“ models, l8 independent V enables were included, resulting in a A cases per- pp I‘(33-ti1natel3’ ' , idin t B and Raudenbush (1992), a co vanable A060 g o ryk mmon rllle of t b all for multivariate analyses is a minimum of 10 cases per variable ineluded in the m ode]. The sample to be analyzed in the present study therefore exceeds this minimum réquiremen t Additive MOdeISI Assessin Direct Effects Amon Variables Additive models were analyzed to determine the independent effects Ofotl‘icer 6 The statistical paCKage used in this study, LIMDEP, requires that the dataset be free from missing data The liStWiSC deletion of cases Inissing scores on any ofthe variables resulted in the sample being eruC. from 3 1 3 to 262 Officers. In order to test whether this changed the sample 0f officers in any meaningfufd way, logistic regl‘i‘fission analyses were run on the full sample (N=313) ‘0 determine whether any OfIhc independent variables significantly predicted the officer being excluded fi‘om the LIMDEP sample (”=2 The dependent variable in these analyses, missing (coded osinclnded meample, 1=missin from s 1 62). was not predicted by any of the independent variables to a statistically Slgmficant eXtent (pi 05) amp e), 91 characteristics (0C), work environment (WE), and social capital (SC) on officer performance of community policing (0}) Tirne and CP Acts). These models test the hypothesis that social capital increases the likelihood that officers will spend time on community policing and provide community policing to citizens. The general equations for these mOdels are presented below. Recall that 0C, WE and SC represent categories of variables. CP Acts = 01+ lHOC) + I3(VVE) +3(SC) + 9 CP Time = a + WOC) + WWI?) + 5(SC) + 0. Significant coefficients for the social capital Val-i ables indicate that $00.1 a1 capital does matter,” and a positive coefficient means that so cial capital significan‘w increases the ukefihood of an officer engaging in connnunity POIicing, A 81-ng fie an‘ positiv e h coefficient would provide evidence that the central hYPOthesiS p ropose d by this teseafc is supported ‘0‘} the data. mdependent variatnes included in the additive models Were tested to determine if multicollinearity Was a problem. A condition number was derived by dividing the largeSt characteristic root from the correlation matrix by the smallest, then taking thé square root ofthat numb er.7 According to Greene (2 000), a condition nmnber less than 20 indicates that the variables are not muItieollinear. The condition number for the matri)K of independent Variables included in the additive models was 4 - 32, indicating that COHCemS regarding multicollinearity are unwarranted for the additive models. A correlation matrix / .— - I Practically, this was accomplished by entering the correlation matrix :01; tile independent variables into a database that was then read into LIMDEP. Commands were thfinfpe 1e ‘0 obtam the characteristic too for the matrix- The Condition number was then derived “by han - ts 92 of all variables is provided in Appendix A Interactive Models: Testin Moderated Cansal Relatio shi s n Moderated causal relationships occm- When the relationship between X (predictor variable) and Y (outcome variable) Varies depending on the value of Z (moderator variable) (Jaccard, Turris, & Wan, 1990). A mod erat or variable affects the direction and/ or strength of the relationship between the Predictor and outcome, so moderators are usefiil for establishing ‘Vvhen certain effects hold” (B at n & Kenny 1986) In Centrest to 0 9 ' mediated relationships (which attempt to account for the relationship b tween X and Y, e or suggest “why or how such effects occur”), moderated relationshj app ropfiate t0 ps are test when there is greater interest in the predictor than the moderat M (1.13th 01-. C relationshipS, on the other hand, suggest that the researcher is most st ed 3;; me intete mediator Variables rather than the predictor variables (Le sacs an -, Officer Charactefi red't‘lior of driven P interest in the current study is social capital, the methOdOIOgical lit upPorts the erature 5 work environment rather than social capital). Since the theor e tic 1 a 1 y— decision to examine moderated causal relationships rather than media ted relationships (Baron & Kennedy, 1986; Hardy, I993; Jaccard, Tunisi, & Wan, 1990)- res ting interactive models enables an evaluation of how the relationship between soQ fa] capit I a and community policing (if one exists) is moderated by officer characteristics an d/or features of their work environment. In Ofder to evaluate the presence of moderated causal relationShiPS, several interaction mOdels were analyzed to determine the j oint effects 0f Pr ed1°Ctor (SC) and moderator (0C, WE) variables on officer performance 0f community Policing. Recall that SC, 0C, 311d WE represent categories of variables- The inter action models include 93 the independent variables from the additive model, with the addition of interaCti on terms representing the social capital dizn eHSions (trust, cooperation, group cohesion, support) moderated by officer characteristics (sex, race, education, tenure) and features of their work envimnmem (department, community Policing assignment, department support of community policing). Descriptive information for the Specific interaction terms to be included in the interactive models is presented in Table 7. Each of the seven interaction models was tested on the two dependent Variables. For ease of presentation, the dependent variable in the equations is referred to as CP. In reality each equation is taste on both CP Time and CP Acts. The general equations for these models are presented below. Social Capital x Officer Characteristics interaction models C? = a + t3(SC)+ (KOCH BONE) + mm x Sex) + e C? = e. +— @(SCV’ BQOCYF BONE) + BCSC x Race) + e C? = u + MSC) + HOG) + BONE) + [3(SC x Education) 4, Q, C? = a + [3(SC) + 5(oc) + [3(WE) + B(SC >< Tenme) _,_ 9 Social Capital x Work Environment interaction models CP = a + [3(SC) + p(oc:)+ [5(WE) + NSC X Dept.) + e CP = a + («3(3) + {3(OC) + 5(WE) + [3(SC >< CPA) + 9 CP = a + [3(SC) + {3(0C) + B(WE) + NSC x Dept SUPPOH ofCP) + 9 Results of the interaction analyses allow important information to be gained along two fronts: Whether the coefficient is significant and Whether the direction of the relationship is positive or negative. For example, if any 0f the 500131 Capital x officer female coefficients are statistically significant, this would mdlcate that the relationship 94 between social capital and community POIiCing depends On the sex of the officerfile., female officers moderate or Change the relationship between social capital and community policing in way that is Significantly different from male officers). If the coefficient is positive, then the lik€1ih00d 0f community policing significantly increases when females have high levels of social capital. If the coefficient is negative, then the likelihood of community policing is significantly reduced when female officers have low levels of social capital. This interaction term can tell us, therefore, Whether the community policing performance of female officers is better understood as a function of their 16V€15 0f social capital being either high or low. In general, the interaction terms indicate when the relationship between social capital and communi ty policing is moderated by officer character-1°Stics and features of their work envirOMent. The interactiOn terms also enable an understanding of whether high or low levels of social Capital are better able to account for community poliCing performance, for Which Officers and under which circumstances. 95 T able "I Measurement and Descri tive Statistics for Interaction Terms. Interaction Term m M w _S_Ll_)_g Trust Variable X Ofc. Female 0 4.00 0.45 1.18 Cooperation Factor x Ofc. Female -2.81 1.41 0.00 0-35 Group Cohesion Factor x Ofc. Female ~2.48 1.05 0.00 0.38 Support Factor X Ofc. Female -2.78 0.79 0.00 0.27 Trust Variable >< Ofc. Minority 0.00 4.00 0,72 1.42 Cooperation Factor >< Ofc. Minority -2.95 1.41 0.00 0.52 Group Cohesion Factor X Ofc. Minority -2.7 7 1.05 0.00 053 Support Factor X Of}; Minority -2.94 0.79 0.00 0.51 TI'USt Variable x OfC Education 2.00 32.00 14.00 655 Cooperation Factor >< Ofc. Education -17.73 0.99 0.00 4.48 Group Cohesion Factor X Ofc. Education -17.06 7.36 -026 4.74 Support Factor x Ofi. Education ’26'17 5‘5 1 ,020 4-73 Trust Variable X Ofc. Tenure 0-00 12400 29.74 25.92 Cooperation Factor >< Ofc. Tenure '65-13 39-51 - 1.20 1 1.90 Group Cohesion Factor X Ofc. Tenure ‘56-33 32-58 0.00 11.05 Support Factor X Ofc Tenure '78.17 24.42 "O.19 1215 Trust Variable X CP Officer 0'00 4'00 1 .22 1.69 Cooperationractor x CP Officer '2-33 1-41 0. 00 060 Group CohesiOn Factor x CP Officer '3'35 1'05 -0.01 0.66 SupportFactOT X C? Officer “2.94 0.79 0.00 057 Trust Variable x Dept. (IPD) 0.00 4.00 1.79 1 77 Cooperation Factor x Dept. (IPD) -295 1-41 0.00 0 '75 Group Cohesion Factor X Dept- (IPD) ‘335 1'05 0-00 0:7 6 Support Factor X DCpt. (IPD) -3.74 O79 0.00 0.79 Trust Variable x Dept. Support 5 80.00 33.57 15.94 Cooperation Factor x Dept. Support 37.22 28-22 0.74 9.99 Group Cohesion Factor x Dept. Support -2994 21.01 0.98 9.79 Support Factor X Dept" Support -40.64 15.76 0.76 9.67 / 5318 96 independent variables inclUded in the seven interactive models were tested to determine if multicollinearity was a PTOblem. These sets of variables change for each model, so a condition number W as derived for each model’s independent Variables. The condition numbers obtained for the sex and race interactive models (1991 and 18.70, respectively), fell just below the value of 20 specified by Greene (2000), indicating that a problematic level of multicollinearity is (not quite) present in these models. The condition numbers derived for the other interactive models, how ever, did exceed the value of 20. Specifically, they were 29.02 for the education model, 2550 for the tenure model, 21.53 for the CPA model, 24.93 for the department mode1, and 31.02 for the department support of CP model. Concerns regarding multicollinearity are therefore warranted for many of the interactive models. Greene (2 000) Pomts out that one cause Of mu 1ticollinearity problems is a shortage of infomation, the solution being to obtain more data. This is not a feasible solution for the Present Study, but is one that future res‘eiiu'chers Should bear in mind. The most practical SOlution suggested by Greene (2000) to deal With PTObIems of muttieomnearity is to dmp the offending variables- This sugges‘ts that the researcher has not been guided by theory during model specification. The models in the entrant research, however, were specified according to theoretical considerations. Silnply remoVing the offending variables (i.€-, the interaction terms) is not a practical SOIution given the focus of the current research. Another method to reduce multico lhnearity is to cwmbine variables that are highly correlated by creating factors or scales. Many of the independent Variables in these models were conceptualized as factors and scales, and 31’ eady have been constructed as such. 97 Readers should be aware that multicollinearity is an issue that is presentin the interactive models, but should also keep in mind that the most PIOHOunccd way that multicollinearity affects results is by making important interaction terms more diffiCUIt to detect statistically (Jaccard, TurriS, & Wan, 1990). The adverse effects are therefore considered to be substantive rather than practical. Multicollinearity does not affect the properties of regression estimates: they remain the best linear unbiased estimates (BLUE) unless perfect coIIi nearity exists (J accard, Tunis, & Wan, 1990). Analytic Methods Dependent variables that involve counts of the number of times a particular act or event occurred, such as those being tested in the current study, Can be found throughOUt social science research. Examples include studies of the number 0 f articles published by academics (Allison & Long. 1 990), the number Offimes that P601) 16 visit the doctor in a certain period (Camaon & Tn'vedi, 1986; Beland, 1980), mortality rates (Hemstrom, 1999., Thouez, 1984; Vlahov et al., 2000), and suicide rates (Aaslamd, Ekeb erg, & Schweder, 2001',Morre11 et al., 1999), among others. Examples specific to Cn'min 010gy include studies of criminal careers (D’Unger Ct 31" 1998; Land, McCaH, & Nagin, 1 9 96), victimizations in American cities (Nelson, 1980) and Britain (OSbOm & T381011]; 1998), police killings (Jacobs & O’Brien, 1998), domestic violence incidents (Shel.man et al., 1992), and homicide counts (Grogger, 1990; Sampson & Raudenbush, 1999). The prevalence of count data in social science research, and criminological research specifically, has led to the widespread application of regression models designed for these data. 98 Regession Models for Count Data There exists aburgeoning literature on regression models for count data that was consulted for this study (D’Unger Ct al., 1998; Greene, 2000; Land, McCall, & Nagin, 1996; Long, 1997; Nelson, 1980; Zorn, 1998). Count outcomes have distributions that are comprised only of nonnegative integerS, and are often skewed toward zero. in other words, a count Variable represents types of events that are generally not experienced by most of the saanle being studied, and is characterized by a nonlinear distribution. Applying the linear regression model (LRM) would therefore produce values that are inefficient, inconsistent, and biased. A more basic concern is that applying the LRM to count data could produce predictions that are 1655 than 261'0- In the context of the current research, applying the LRM could result in values indicating a negative number of citizens served, or a negative amount of time spent on community policing, both of which are nonsensical. Fortunately there €Xists a group of regression models that have been deveIOped particularly for count outcomes. In contTaSt to applying the LRM, these regression models make the estimation ofcount models more efficient, more consistent, and less biased. The most basic of these models is the Porsson regreSSIOIl model (PRM), Which has two defining assumPtions, The first assumption is that the events being Counted are independent of each other. When they are not, the process is known as CO" tagion and the assumption of independence has been violated. This can 060111“ in two WaYs. First, if the probability of performing community policing is the same for all officers but depends upon prior Community policing performance, then the events are depencleIlt upon each other- Secorxd, if the probability of performing community Policing is Constant over 99 time,but is not the same for all OffiCCI'S, then contagion has occurred. T116 second assumption for the PRM is that the conditional mean of the outcome is equal to the conditional variance. Equidisper-Sion is another term for the equality 0f the mean and variance assumption. In practice both of these assumptions are often violated, leading to the development of additional regression models for count data. W The negative binomial regression Il’IOde1 (NBRM) builds Upon the PRM design but provides added flexibility by relaxing the. assumptionsof independence and equidispersion that can limit the applicability of the PRM. Contagion v i olates the assumption of independence required for PRM and can result in a failure of the model to fit the data. Recall that one Way contagion can occur is via heterogeneity in the rate among the individuals being studied; failure to account for this heterogeneity can contribute to overdispersion in the marginal distribution (see Long, 1997, p. 221). The PRM includes Observed heterogeneity to accol—lnt for a different likelihOOd of me count Outcome across individuals, but the NBRM also in 0111 des unobserved hetefogeneity in an attempt to more completely address the prob Item of contagion. With cross-sectional data, however, it is impossible to state definitively whether the observed counts are produCtS 0f“apparent” contagion “'6" addressed by including unobserved heterogeneity), or “true” contagion. It is a limitation ofthe NBRM, but it is less of a concem compared to the PRM. The primary benefit of the NBRM is that it was designed to address es the issue of overdispersion that often exists in count data as a result of the variance exceeding the mean- In “real life” it is rare that the variance equals the mean; the measures of community policing in the current study also failed to meet this requirement. The 100 NBRM allows for the estimation Of overdispersed count Vaziables by adding a parameter that allows the conditional variance t0 exceed the conditional mean, known as the dispersion parameter, or alpha. When the alpha coefficient equals zero, the central assumption of PRM has not been Violated and therefore PRM is suited to the data. Another way of stating this is that the NBRM reduces to the PRM when the alpha coefficient is equal to zero. Conversely, when the alpha coefficient is significantly different fi'om zero, the NBRM is a more suitable model than the PRM because the data are substantially over-dispersed. Erroneously applying the PRM to overdispersed data will produce estimates that exaggerate the significance of the independent variables. The NBRM avoids this problem by including the dispersion paramaer’ thereby achieVing more accurate indications of the statistical importance of the Vatiab 165. Even among the specially designed regression models for count outcomes there are substantial repercussions for employing a model wrongly suited to the data. When attempting to model count data, it is therefore imperative to determine whether overdispersion exists in the data to a significant degree. The statistical Package L IMDEP computes alpha, the dispeI'Sion parameter, for the NBRM. The PrOCess of estimating the NBRM in LIMDEP is threefold: first the statistical software generates prediction values using linear re gressiOn (0L5), then uses these values to eStiInate the pRM, then uses the PRM values to estimate the NBRM. If the t-test 0f the alpha parameter provided in the NBRM is statistically significant (p < .05) this is evidence 0 fa Significant amount of overdispersion. A second way to test for overdispersion is to Compare the log—likelihoods of the PRM and the NBRM. The Log Ratio (LR) test (see Long, 1997, p- 237) compares the log-likelihood of the two models in order to determine lOl which is better (i -e., closer to zerO)- Comparing the Z-Scores for the coefficien ts between the two models is a third method 0f testing for overdispersion When a significant amount of overdispersion is present, the z-scores are generally smaller for the NBRM than the PRM. Recall that when the PRM is wrongly applied the z-scores are inflated and the Significance of the variables is exaggerated. The NBRM, on the other hand, more precisely estimates the z-scores in overdispersed data, allowing the researcher to draw more accurate conclusions. All three indicators of overdispersion are evident in the models for both CP Acts and CP Time, indicating that PRM is an ill-suit ed regression model for these data. WW An additional issue raised with count models is the prediction of zeros. A large number of Zero counts can facilitate overdispersion in the data because when the dependent variable is skewed toward zero it is not possible to achieve eclualit)’ between the mean and variance s Typically, the PRM as well as the NBRM under-pre dict the amount of zeros in the dependent variable These models would therefore be inefficient at predicting those officers that Spend zero minutes per shift on community policing activities, or who provide zero acts of Conlmunily policing per citizen encountered. Zero—inflated models for count outcomes address this problem by modeling the predicted zeros specifically. In other words, zero.1-nfla ted models assume that a different process occurs for officers who perform no Cominum'ty policing compared to officers who perform some community policing. Similar to evaluating the alpha parameter to determine whether the PRM or the NBRM is more suited to the data, the tau parameter evaluates whether the data should be modeled With a zero-inflated count model. prrior analyses demonstrate that the NBRM is more suited 102 to the data than me PRM, as was evident for both CP Time and CP Acts, then tau indicates whether the data would be even better suited to a zero—inflated NBRM (known as ZINE). LIMI)EP thus adds a fourth stage (ZINB) to the threefold process described earlier (OLS, PRM, NBRM). A significant (p < .05) t-test of the tau coefficient indicates that the ZINB is a more efficient model and better at predicting zeros in the outcome measure compared to the standard NBRM. In the present study, tau was significant for all the CP Time models, but was not once significant for the CP Acts models. The ZINB is therefore a more appropriate regression model for CP Time compared to the NBRM. This is n 0 t a surprising result given that the majority of officers did not engage in any minutes of community policing activity per shifi (i- e-, 165 of 262 officers had scores of zero). AS an example, results for the CP Time additive model indicate that the ZINB predicted 162 Qf 165 actual zero scores, whereas the NBRM predicted 158 of 165, and the PRM pre di ct ed 0 of 165. Each successive model is better able to account for the large number of Zeros present in the data. Similar improvements in the prediction of zero scores were Observed for all 0ftbe C? Time models, and the tan parameter was statistically significant for all ofthe CP Time models. For CP Acts, on the other hand, tau was never significant and the prediCtion of zeros never improved with the ZINE. When we consider that most officers did PI‘OVidc at least one act of community policing per citizen encountered (i.e., only 23 of 252 officers had scores of zero), it makes intuitive sense that the NBRM is bett er suited to CF Acts than is the 2%, In conclusion, results from LIIVEDEP indicate that NBRM is the best suited lhodel for Cp Acts, whereas ZINB is the best suited model for CP Time. 103 CHAPTER 6 Table 8 presents the correlation analysis of the independent and dependent variables, What is most notable from this table is the lack of significant findingsi only dent four 0f 28 relationships tested reached a level of statistical significance. E3011 depen endent Variables. variable is significantly (p <.01) associated with tvvo of the 14 indep - On Female officers engage in more community pelicing as measured by C? Tune. . . . — Olvm a or av erage they spent 23 minutes per shift engaged in crime prevention, 1)“)me S g attending community meetings, While male officers spent less that] 1 0 minutes per Shili on these activities. Indianapolis officers performed significantly less community policing, as measured by bOlh CP Time and CP Acts, compared to St- Petersburg office I S. For example, IPD officers spent about eight minutes per Shin on Commum' . 13’ pollcin g (compared to 18 minutes per shift for SPD officers) and provided two acts 0 . - 1° Community pohcmg per citizen (compared to more than three aCtS per Citizen provided by S PD officers). Officer tenure was also negatively related to the number of CP Ac tS per CltlZen encountered. As tenure increases, the number 0f community p01icing acts per - ~ A CltlZen decreases. Of the f ' 'ficant independent variables, then, our srgm only one (Officer female) increased the likelihood of community policing occum'ng — the Other thre 'gnif 1 e 51 103m y reduced its likelihood. 104 Table 8 Bivariate Correlates of Communit Policin . W per shift ’ p Officer Characteristics Offi cer Female 0-1 78 0.004 Officer Non-white -0.038 0.536 Officer Education -0.0l 3 0,839 Officer Tenure -0.054 0.388 CP Training Scale 0.008 0.895 Work En viron ment Department app) -0.185 0.003 Day Shifi -0.01 0.899 CPA 0.101 0.102 Beat Problems Scale -0.03 0.626 Dept. Pro-CF Scale -0016 0.792 Socia\ Capit‘d TrustVariab\e 0.057 0.360 CooperationFactor -0.01 0873 Group Cohesion Factor 0.024 0.697 Support Factor -0.014 0.827 N=262 cp Acts m r P 0.074 - 0-234 -0052 0-4 0.075 0-228 -0.174 0.005 -0.084 0.177 0 -0218 -0104 0825 0.064 0 447 -0047 ' 67 -0069 0‘2 0.017 0.779 -0072 0.243 -0.042 0.502 0.053 0.396 \ Table 9 presents the correlations between the interaction terms and the tw 0 dependent measures. Trust x IPD was negatively associated with both Cp Tim e and CP Acts, indicating that officers who work in Indianapolis and have low levels oftru t S tend to engage in significantly less community policing per shift and provide significant] Y fewer community policing acts per citizen. Another way of interpreting this finding i s that the difference in community poliCing performance between IPD and SP D is a function ofIPD officers «reporting less trust in their supervisors. For ex le almost alhp , one 105 in four lPD officers disagreed that they trusted their sapervisor (2 1 , 5%), compared to only 12.5% of SPD officers. This difference in trust between the departments, however, did not reach the conventional level of statistical significance )8 (3, N = 262) = 6.04, p = . 1 l 0. This is one indication of the iInportance of computing interaction terms: when trust and deparunent are combined they Share a sigtnficam relationship with community policing, even though trust levels did n0t differ to a significant degree between departments. . . . . ther Trust x Tenure was negatively assoc1ated with CP Acts per Citizen- In 0 . . . ' their words, officers vv1th a lot of time on the Job who reported low levels of “St in of referrals to ovided the supervisors were less likely to engage in providing comfort, informafion’ dlfizens they encountered. Officers with less than two years on the job pr highest mean C? Acts (2.98), While ofiicers with 1 8 year 5 01' more on the job PIOVided the lowest mean C? Acts (1 .33). Mean CP Acts steadily decrease as Officer tenure increases, and the difference in community policing based on t(inure categories was also statistically significant, F (4, N = 262) = 3.212, p < .02. In short, tenure direCtIy impact the amount of community policing performed, and this relationship is also Significanuy S moderated by the amount of trust an officer has in his or her supervisor. Trust x Female was positively related to GP Time, indicating that female offio with high levels of trust tend to engage in more COIanlity POIiCing per shift. There :rs also a significant difference between the amount 0f trust reported by male and female as officers, x2 (3, N = 262) = 795, P < -05 - Four times as many male officers reported that they did not trust their supervisors (201 % compared to 5.2%). About 950/ f fem l o O a e officers, on the other hand, reported that they trusted their supervisors Th high I - e er evel 106 Of “390““ trust among female Officers is therefore one potential epraflatiOH for why they engage in significantly more Community policing per shjfi than their male counterparts. The four significant interaction terms mirror the significant findings presented in Table 8- We can infer from the bivariate results that trust is influencing the relationships between Officer Female, Officer Tenure, IPD and community policing. One intereSting feature revealed by the interaction term analyses is that Trust, rather than Cooperation, Group Cohesion, or Support, is the social capital dimension that appears to make a . _ , ble in difference, albelt shght. Overall, the findings presented in Tables 8 and 9 are nota ‘ ° - - tion the ”dc 0f Slgnlficant 1' elationsthS between the independent variables, the mterac terms, and the dependent measures. Of the 84 biv an'ate relationships tested (42 per dependent variable), six were statistically significant. In other Words, only one in every 14 relationships met the conventional level of significance. 107 Table 9 Bivariate Correlates of Com uni Policin — Interaction Term; CP Time per shift CP Acts per citizen r p r P Trust Variable X Ofc. Female 0.190 0.002 001333 353: Cooperation Factor X Ofc. Female -0.02 0.750 ‘0-103 0097 Group Cohesion Factor X Ofc. Female 0.00 0.904 ‘0‘011 0.863 Support Factor X Ofc. Female -0.08 0.220 0- 0 033 0.592 Trust Variable X Ofc. Minority '0.02 0780 i '055 0.376 cooperation Factor X Ofc. Minority . 0.030 0.628 0' 015 0.803 Group Cohesion Factor X Ofc. Minority -0-03 0-682 ’8‘019 0.756 Support Factor >< Ofc- Education . 0.032 0.608 968090 0.1434] Cooperation Factor X Ofc. Education . -0- ()2 0.693 $.05 0.3 9 Group Cohesion Factor X Ofc.. Education 0-033 3333 0.049 0-42 Support Factor X Ofc. Education 0.014 - 0 686 .0148 0.017 TrustVariable X Ofc. Tenure -0-03 0-701 0 046 0.461 CooperationFactor x Ofc. Tenure 0.024 0.496 ‘ . O 940 Group CohesiQnFactof X Ofc. Tenure 0.042 0‘795 0.005 ' Support Factor X Ofc. Tenure 0.016 ' 0.045 0.470 Trust Variable X CP Assignment 0.079 0203 0.094 0.129 Cooperation Factor X CP Assignment 0-053 0396 0.037 0 548 Group Cohesion Factor X CP Assignment (1032 0609 -029 0643 Support Factor X CP Assignment ‘0- 12 0051 0.102 0 '100 Trust Variable >< Dept. (IPD) '0-15 (“us 41199 0 00 Cooperation Factor X Dept (IPD) ‘0‘ 04 0'494 ‘0-044 0 '47 1 Group Cohesion Factor X Dept. (IPD) 0028 0‘656 0-008 0 89: Support Factor X Dept. (IPD) 0005 0-936 0.039 0532 Trust Variable x Dept. Support 0016 “-791 ~0.03g 0.542 Cooperation Factor X Dept. Support Of CP ’0'002 8 '71 1 ‘0-052 0.401 Grp. Cohesion Factor X Dept. Support Of CP (3)03 0'338 0037 0550 support Factor X Dept. Support of CP ' - - 6 0.046 0.460 1 \__\ N=262 108 Multivariate Analyses Additive Mo dels Table 10 presents the findings 1mm the Additive Models. The Zero-Inflated Negative Binomial regression model (ZINB) was used to regress officer characteristlcs, work environment, and social capital Variables on (3}) Time per shift. The Negative t of Binomial regression model (NBRM) Was used to regress CP Acts on the same 56 . . - ' 11 independent variables. Once agam what 15 notable is the overall lack of statistica y - . . . - blCS were significant predictors of community policmg. None of the independent V3113 . lose at? 5 significant predictors of CP Time (although the departmait variable came C . . , p Acts .087), but the model itself is statistically significant, The regressron {of C ' - - - ' 5 receiving produced three significant predictors that all reduce the llkellhOOd of citizen acts of continuum}l Policing. FirSt, as officer tenure increases, the likelihood 0f CP Ads decreases significantly (but not necessarily substantially ’ only by 3%),8 Second, officers having a community policing assignment reduces the expeCted number of CP 0 ' ' b1 constant. Finally, bein an Acts by 30 /o holding all other vana es g 1P D Officer decreases the expected number of CP Acts by 47%, holding all other variables constant N - one of the social capital dimensions were significant predictors of either dependent var-lab] e. _‘_ 8 ‘ forming beta coefficients from Poiss - Long (1997) prowdes a formula for tans . on or he . . . regression into percentages for am: of interpretation (see page 228). The formula is (Elana/C 3121;321:111 The formula was computed for all sigmficant (p <.05) coefficrents. 0 [C P - D. 109 Table l() Additive Models for Communi POIiCiDO Variables. CP Time ZINB M 11518] 1—3- Si P 3. E 3 Constant 2.49 1.5 7 0_1—1 3 2.23 0.58 0000 Officer Characteristics 072 Officer Female 0- 56 0.34 0,105 0.27 0.15 0-334 Officer Non-white 0.05 0.42 0,901 -0.14 0.15 3-442 Officer Education 0.05 0.10 0.587 0.03 0'04 0'004 Otficer Tenure 0.01 0.03 0.721 -0.03 0'01 ' 8 CP Trarmng Scale 0.03 0.04 0.456 0.00 - Work Environ ment 0 16 0.000 Department (IPD) -0.55 0.32 0.087 -0.63 0' 14 0989 Day Shin 0.15 0.35 0.675 0.00 0'18 0.038 C? Assignment —o.04 0.41 0.922 -o.36 0’02 0,480 BeatProblems Scale -o.01 0.06 0.816 -001 0' 02 0.581 Dept.Pro-CP Scale 0.01 0.05 0.826 -001 ' Socia\ Ca “3‘ Trust Variable 0.07 0.32 0.829 -012 0.13 0.388 CooperationFactor -0.08 0.18 - 0.642 ~0.07 0.05 0.196 Group Cohesion Factor 0.19 0.19 0.299 ~O. 02 0 07 0 826 _ 4 ' ‘ Support Factor 0.27 0.26 0.29 0.06 0.13 0.213 Alpha 1.09 0.22 0.000 0.27 O Tau 0.13 0.04 0.002 -07 0.000 Model Fit Log-L 693.33 108-L -503.52 Vuong 10-91 Chi-sq. 52.86 DP“ 2 Dt“ 1 Sig. Level 0000 Sig. Level 0.000 N=262 Notes: * Compared to the Poisson regression model. Alpha compares Poisson to Negative Binomial (significance indicates a better fit ofth compares the NB model with the Zero-Inflated NB model (significance indicates a be e NB model). Tau model). ZINB regression models were tested on both dependent measures, Tau indi “er fit 0f the ZINB always better than NB for CP Time, but not once improved the fit for CP ACtS. A V Gated that ZINB Was 1.96 favors the ZINB over the NB model. “Orig stat1snc less than 110 Interactive Models Table 11 presents the results of the interactive models for CP Time and Table 12, presents the results for CP Acts. Interaction terms (created with the social capital dimensions and various officer Characteristic and Work environment variables) were included along with the variables from the additive models in order to assess the extent to WhiCh they might enhance our eXplanation of community policing performance. AS these tables make readily apparent, the interaction terms do not contribute much explanatory power to the originaI models. Only one of the 56 interaction coefficients (28 per dependent meas ure) Was a significant predictor of community policing- . . 1 Su Po“ Of The Significant interaction term, Group Cohesion Factor x Bellman p Comurutv Policing, reduces the likelihood of CP Time per shift by 1% Officers Who Perceived they were part of a cohesive group, and who also Perceived a high degree Of departmental support of community policing, spent significantly leSS time per Shlfi engaged in community policing. Qne of the original variables Was 3130 Significant in this model: Group Cohesion. Officers who scored high on the group cohesion factor were more likely to spend time engaged in community policing; they increased t1) e likelih of community policing by 239%, holding all other variables conStant. Together the 00d variables indicate that group cohesion will increase the likelihood of CP Time, b1“ n: when a high degree of departmental support of community Policing is also present. Perhaps group cohesion is only effeCtiV’e at increasing officer Productivity precisely When departmental support is lacking. Under these circumstances, officers who want to practice community policing may more heavily rely on support from their peers or workgroup because they cannot rely on similar support from th e department. Overall, it 1 1 1 is important to place this finding in the context of a general pattem which suggests that social capital, whether in interaCtion form or not, does not help us understand or predict officer performance of community POIiCil'lg. Additionally, the Likelihood Ratio test indicated that the interaction model With the significant term did not PTOVide Significantly more information compared to the . . . . ' t additive model. One way to judge the lmPOI'tance of a significant interaction term 15 0 . . does determine whether the model including the interaction term fits the data better than . . . __, - 93 .33 + the Original model without the interaction term. The Log Ratio Test [LR " 2 ( 5 ed the critical 589.44) = 7.88 3 indicated that the obtained chi-square (7.78) did not ex“ . . . . \) so moludmg chi-square (9-49, Wlth 4 degrees of freedom at the -05 Significance leVe ’ ' - . , - udin mem- the interaction terms did not prOVide Significantly more mformatl on than excl g in other WOYdS, the two models were statistically indistinguishab le, despite the presence of one significant interaction term, This casts further doubt on the iInpo l cc of this finding. In conclusion, the results from Tables 1 1 and 12 Should be ConSl'dered notable for their uniformity of effect: nil. 112 Tame 11 Interaction Models - Zero—Inflated Negative Binomial Regression on Community olicino Time- _P_,..——-——--=-——-—— Sex Tenure Race Education Constant Trust Variable X Ofc. Female Cooperation Factor X Ofc. Female Group Cohesion Factor X Ofc. Female Support Factor X Ofc. Female Alpha Tau Log-Likelihood/V uong/ Si g. Level Constant Trust Variable X Ofc. Minority Cooperation Factor X Ofc. Minority Group Cohesion Factor X Ofc. Minority Support Factor X Ofc. Minority Alpha Tau Lo g-Likelihood/V uong/ Si g. Level Constant Trust Variable X Ofc. Education Cooperation Factor X Ofc. Education Group Cohesion Factor X Ofc. Education Support Factor X Ofc. Education Alpha Tau Log-Likelihood/V uong/ Si g. Level Constant Trust Variable X Ofc. Tenure Cooperation Factor X Ofc. Tenure Group Cohesion Factor X Ofc. Tenure Support Factor X Ofc. Tenure Alpha Tau Log-Likelihood/V uong/ Si g. Level 113 CP Time per shift B 2.86 0.40 0.28 0.17 -0.71 1.03 0.14 -591.48 2.82 0.15 0.16 -0.66 0.72 1.01 0.13 -589.79 -2.53 -0.37 0.05 0.00 0.24 1.02 0.14 -591.43 2.83 0.03 0.02 0.03 0.00 1.11 0.12 -592.6 _S_E 1.60 1.19 0.60 0.46 1.38 0.21 0.04 10.91 1.66 1.54 0.60 0.39 0.97 0.20 0.04 11.25 3.59 0.24 0.14 0.14 0.23 0.20 0.04 11.14 3.27 0.10 0.04 0.05 0.08 0.23 0.04 10.05 2 0.073 0.735 0.637 0.711 0.604 0.000 0.001 0.000 0.089 0.921 0.795 0.089 0.455 0.000 0.002 0.000 0.480 0.125 0.740 0.995 0.294 0.000 0.001 0.000 0.387 0.748 0.656 0.598 0.968 0.000 0.005 0.000 Table 11 (cont’d), CP. Constant 2.51 1.15 0.249 ASSIgn ment Female“ 0.77 0.37 0.037 Trust Variable X CP Assignment -0.02 0.75 0.977 Cooperation Factor X CP Assignment 0.43 0.44 0.322 Group Cohesion Factor X CP Assignment -0.07 0.38 0.857 Support Factor X CP Assignment -0.67 0.63 0.289 Alpha 1 .04 0.22 0.000 Tau 0.12 0.04 0.006 Log-Likelihood/V uong/ Si g. Level -5 89.61 10.83 0.000 DeP al’tment Constant 1.82 2.12 0.392 Trust Variable X Dept. (IPD) -0.5 0.81 0.534 C00peration Factor X Dept. (IPD) -0.53 0.42 0.211 Group Cohesion Factor X Dept. (IPD) 0.40 0.42 0.340 Support Factor X Dept. (IPD) 0.37 0.64 0.566 Alpha 1.02 0.21 0.000 Tau 0.14 0.04 0.001 Log-Likelihood/V uong/ Sig. Level -S90.63 10.97 0.000 Dept. Support Constant -0.51 3.70 0.891 of CP Group Cohesion Factor 1.22 0.54 0.024 Trust Variable X Dept. Support -0.11 0.11 0.320 Cooperation Factor X Dept. Support 0.03 0.71 0.712 Group Cohesion Factor X Dept. Support -0.12 0.06 0.050 Support Factor X Dept. Support 0.14 0.11 0.197 Alpha 1 .00 0.20 0.000 Tau 0.13 0.04 0.002 Log-Likelihood/V uong/ Sig. Level -5 89.44 1 1.34 0.000 N=262 N otes: *Each interaction model was analyzed with the variables included in the additive model for CP Tune, but only those variables that reached statistical significance are presented in the table. ** This Variable also came close to attaining statistical significance in the following models: Race (p =.06), Department (p =07), Dept. Support of CP (p =.07). 114 Table 12 Interaction Models — Negative Binomial Regression on Community Policing Acts.* Sex Race Education Tenure Constant Trust Variable X Ofc. Female Cooperation Factor X Ofc. Female Group Cohesion Factor X Ofc. Female Support Factor X Ofc. Female Alpha Log-Likelihood/Chi-Squared/Sig. Level Constant Trust Variable X Ofc. Minority Cooperation Factor X Ofc. Minority Group Cohesion Factor X Ofc. Minority Support Factor X Ofc. Minority Alpha Log-Likelihood/Chi-Squared/Sig. Level Constant Trust Variable X Ofc. Education Cooperation Factor X Ofc. Education Group Cohesion Factor X Ofc. Education Support Factor X Ofc. Education Alpha Log-Likelihood/Chi-Squared/Sig. Level Constant Trust Variable X Ofc. Tenure Cooperation Factor X Ofc. Tenure Group Cohesion Factor X Ofc. Tenure Support Factor X Ofc. Tenure Alpha Log-Likelihood/Chi-Squared/Sig. Level 115 B 2.33 0.14 -0.04 -0.03 0.00 0.27 -503.24 2.68 0.51 0.19 -0.13 -0.4 0.26 -500.47 2.01 -0.02 -0.01 -0.02 0.53 0.27 -503.08 2.77 0.02 0.00 0.00 -0.01 0.27 —502.86 S_E 0.61 0.40 0.20 0.18 0.37 0.07 51.84 0.60 0.40 0.15 0.16 0.32 0.07 48.41 1.66 0.10 0.04 0.05 0.10 0.07 52.79 0.87 0.02 0.01 0.01 0.02 0.07 52.31 2 0.000 0.718 0.818 0.861 0.987 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.202 0.205 0.419 0.210 0.000 0.000 0.226 0.862 0.770 0.696 0.957 0.000 0.000 0.001 0.465 0.637 0.802 0.609 0.000 . 0.000 Table 12 (cont’d). CP Constant 2.80 0.68 0.000 Assignment Trust Variable X CP Assignment 0.37 0.28 0.188 Cooperation Factor X CP Assignment 0.12 0.12 0.324 Group Cohesion Factor X CP Assignment 0.01 0.15 0.941 Support Factor X CP Assignment -0.15 0.26 0.558 Alpha 0.26 0.07 0.000 Log-Likelihood/Chi-Squared/Sig. Level -500.64 47.50 0.000 Dept. Constant 2.02 0.77 0.008 Trust Variable X Dept. (IPD) -0.13 0.27 0.636 Cooperation Factor X Dept. (IPD) -0.1 0.11 0.341 Group Cohesion Factor X Dept. (IPD) 0.06 0.14 0.666 Support Factor X Dept. (IPD) , 0.15 0.26 0.552 Alpha 0.27 0.07 0.000 Lo g-Likelihood/ Chi-Squared/ Si g. Level -502.77 51.25 0.000 Dept. Support Constant 2.98 1.55 0.055 of CP Trust Variable X Dept. Support 0.02 0.05 0.686 Cooperation Factor X Dept. Support 0.01 0.02 0.657 Group Cohesion Factor X Dept. Support 0.01 0.02 0.554 Support Factor X Dept. Support 0.01 0.05 0.870 Alpha 0.26 0.07 0.000 Log-Likelihood/Chi-Squared/ Sig. Level -501.87 51.00 0.000 N=262 Notes: * Each interaction model was analyzed with the variables included in the additive models. ** Significant coefficients from the additive model (tenure, CPA, IPD) also had the same sign and significance in these models, with the exception of tenure in the tenure interaction model, CPA in the CPA interaction model, and IPD in the department interaction model. 116 Summary of Findings Table 13 presents a summary of the statistically significant coefficients produced by the bivariate and multivariate analyses. Table 13 Summm of Findings. Variable Bivariate Multivariate Female Officer + CP Time Female Officer X Trust + CP Time IPD - CP Time - CP Acts - CP Acts IPD X Trust ‘— CP Time " CP Acts Tenure - CP Acts - CP Acts Tenure X Trust - CP Acts CP Assignment - CP Acts Group Cohesion Factor + CP Time Group Cohesion Factor X - CP Time Dept. Support of CP What should be given primary consideration from the results presented is the overall lack of significant findings produced by the bivariate analyses, the additive models, the interactive models, and the analyses of district effects. Of the 352 regression coefficients produced in these analyses, only about 5% reached the conventional level of significance. The findings related to social capital and community policing are notable in their consistent pattern of unimportance. Of the three categories of independent variables (officer characteristics, work environment, and social capital), the social capital group by 117 far offered the least explanatory power. The most consistent significant findings produced in the analyses were contributed by officer characteristics and features of their work environments. Moreover, the variables that were significant tended to reduce, rather than increase, the expected amount of community policing time per shifi or the number of community policing acts per citizen performed by the officers. The most consistent significant result (both statistically and substantively) was the organizational environment in which the officer worked. This was originally conceptualized at the department level. Officers working in Indianapolis were consistently found to produce fewer community policing minutes per shift and acts per citizen compared to officers working in St. Petersburg. The substantial difference in community policing performance between departments provides a strong indication of the importance that organizational factors play in the likelihood of community policing being performed. The results of these analyses also reveal that different conceptualizations of community policing can lead to different results. The additive model for community policing time did not produce any significant results, but female officers spent significantly more time on community policing compared to their male counterparts in one of the interaction models. Additionally, another interaction model revealed that as officers’ perceptions of group cohesion increased so did the amount of time they engaged in community policing activities.9 For the other dependent variable, the additive model 9 While there was one significant interaction term for a CP Time model (group cohesion X dept. support of CP), recall that the LR test showed that the interaction model did not provide significantly more information than did the original model. The significance of interaction effects should be judged in terms of the entire research process; the general pattern of non-significance makes this finding especially dubious. 118 produced several significant findings while there were no significant findings for the interaction models. Officer tenure and CPA consistently reduced the expected number of CP Acts per citizen, holding other variables constant. A strong department effect also emerged: officers in Indianapolis were significantly less likely to provide acts of comfort, referrals, and information to citizens compared to officers in St. Petersburg. The next chapter puts these findings in context of the existing literature on social capital, community policing, and police organizations. Implications for promoting community policing activities across disparate organizational environments are discussed. 119 CHAPTER 7 ISSUES RAISED BY THE RESEARCH FINDINGS The goal of this chapter is to discuss the issues raised by the present study. These are grouped into two sections. The first section of this chapter describes the substantive issues raised by the findings from the statistical analyses. The second section provides an overview of the methodological issues relevant to the current study as well as directions for future research investigating officer performance of community policing within a social capital framework. Substantive Issues Research Questions Revisited Let us first examine how the results from the statistical analyses provide answers to the research questions presented in the introduction. The first question, “Is social capital related to how police officers perform their jobs?” and its follow-up question, “Specifically, what is the relationship between levels of social capital and officers’ engagement in community policing activities?” can be answered by referring to the bivariate results, as they provide an indication of whether a significant direct relationship is present. The multivariate results are used to answer the remaining research questions, as they deal with the social capital-community policing relationship in the context of other moderating variables. Bivariate results did not indicate a direct relationship between social capital and community policing. However, trust was revealed as an important dimension of social 120 capital through the presence of several interaction terms that were significantly related to the measures of community policing. Specifically, the relationship between trust and community policing was moderated by gender, department, and tenure. Levels of trust significantly increased community policing time per shift for officers who are female. This means that trust in supervisors is more relevant to female officers’ performance of community policing. Male officers engaged in community policing regardless of their levels of trust. Levels of trust were negatively related to both community policing time and community policing acts for officers working in Indianapolis. In other words, trust influenced officers’ performance of community policing in [PD but not SPD. The direction of the relationship suggests that trust makes community policing in Indianapolis less likely. This could be viewed as a negative outcome of social capital — perhaps having trust in their supervisors allowed officers to “get away” with performing less community policing. A similar relationship emerged for tenure and trust. Levels of trust decreased acts of community policing per citizen for experienced officers. When officers had a lot of time on the job, they tended to have higher levels of trust in their supervisors, and they used this trust to more easily circumvent the community policing mandate. The likelihood that rookies would engage in community policing, on the other hand, was unaffected by the amount of trust they had in their supervisors. These results demonstrate that of the four social capital dimensions, trust played an important role at the bivariate level. It affected community policing performance in the expected direction only for female officers: high levels of trust promoted rather than hindered their engagement in community policing. For officers in Indianapolis and with 121 high tenure, however, high levels of trust decreased the likelihood they would engage in community policing. The second research question addressed by the current research, “What is the relative contribution of police social capital in a model that also includes characteristics of the individual officer and their work environment?” can be answered in a straightforward manner: none. The quality of officers’ relationships with their peers and supervisors did not influence whether officers spent time on community policing or provided community policing acts to citizens, controlling for officer characteristics and features of their work environments. Given the wealth of literature pointing to the potential importance of social capital in understanding police behavior, how can the current results be explained? Aside from any methodological limitations that may have contributed to the null findings (discussed in the next section), why would levels of trust, cooperation, group cohesion, and social support among police not matter to community policing performance? One explanation is that the relationships that are really important to officers Wanting to engage in community policing are not police relationships, but rather citizen relationships. This study did not provide information on the extent to which officers Were networked into relationships in the community. The four social capital dimensions OF trust, support, cooperation, and group cohesion could be viewed as especially irrlDortant elements of relationships between officers and citizens. Given that the central tel-1 %t of the community policing philosophy is that police and citizens should work to g ether to reduce crime and increase safety in communities, describing the qualities of the: Se relationships could provide an important explanation of community policing 122 performance. Future researchers should consider assessing community policing performance in terms of networks of relationships within police organizations, within other relevant agencies, and within the citizenry, as well as relationships that reach across these different groups. Another way to interpret the finding that “social capital does not matter” is in positive terms. Some might argue that police performance should not be dependent upon levels of police social capital. In other words, police should engage in community policing regardless of whether they have relationships with their peers and supervisors that are rich in trust, cooperation, support and/or group cohesion. Police officers should do their jobs no matter what their level of resources. This interpretation relies on a model of policing that is individualistic and self-determined; officers’ performance is viewed as based solely on their own will, motivation, and determination. It removes consideration of environmental characteristics which have been shown to play an important role in behavioral outcomes among officers. The third question, “Do officer characteristics, such as their sex, race, education, of tenure moderate the relationship between social capital and community policing?” can b e answered by looking at the impact of the OC X SC interaction terms on the regression m QCiels of community policing time and community policing acts. No significant intéraction terms were revealed by the analyses. So while officer characteristics might Sigl'iificantly moderate the relationship between social capital and community policing at the bivariate level, once other relevant factors are controlled these effects disappear. Th§re was one interesting direct relationship among these variables: as officer tenure Incl-eased, the amount of community policing acts per citizen decreased. This replicates 123 the finding of most other research indicating that as years of experience increase, productivity decreases (Bittner, 1990; DeJong, Mastrofski, & Parks, 2001; Muir, 1977; Roberg, Crank & Kuykendall, 2000; Stalans & Firm, 1995). The fourth question, “Do features of officers’ work environment, such as their department and their perceptions of the department’s support for community policing, . moderate the relationship between social capital and community policing?” is answered by referring to the Work Environment X Social Capital interaction terms in the regression models for community policing time and community policing acts. Recall that one social capital interaction term significantly decreased the amount of community policing time per shift: Group Cohesion X Department Support of Community Policing. This was interpreted as indicating that a high level of group cohesion decreased time spent in community policing when a high degree of departmental support of community policing was also present. The importance of this finding is doubtful given the general pattern of non-importance of social capital, and the Log Ratio test that revealed that the interaction model did not provide significantly more information than the additive model (i.e., the interaction term did not sigrificantly increase, in a statistical sense, our understanding of Community policing time). Two other direct relationships are also revealed by this group of variables: CO Immunity Policing Assignment and Department. First, officers assigned as community po 1 icing specialists provided significantly fewer acts of community policing per citizen th Ext-:1 did regular patrol officers. This finding is inconsistent with original expectations that community policing officers would provide more community policing acts. It does, hQ‘OVever, replicate another analysis of data from the POPN that found that community 124 policing officers in both sites spent less time in encounters with citizens than did patrol generalists (Parks, et al. 1999). These officers had more discretion than general patrol officers, and they used it to engage in less “face time” with the public, or to spend more time with citizens of higher status. This study generates a similar conclusion about how community policing and patrol officers tend to do their jobs. Community policing officers provided significantly fewer acts of community policing to citizens, and regarding community policing time per shift there was no difference between the two groups of officers. The fourth research question does draw our attention to the most consistent finding produced by the current study: the importance of officers’ work environment. Specifically, the department where the officer works exerts a substantial impact on the expected amount of community policing time and community policing acts performed by officers, controlling for the effects of the other independent variables. To state it bluntly, officers in Indianapolis were far less likely to engage in community policing time or acts Compared to their counterparts working in St. Petersburg. What organizational factors relevant to community policing can account for such a pronounced difference between the practice of community policing in these two departments? The community policing 1i tel-attire offers several explanations as to organizational variables that may affect officer p erformance of community policing. These are discussed below. W “Providing leadership and vision is an important part of any organizational Change strategy” (Skogan & Hartnett, 1997, p. 91). Top police administrators are S‘Z‘IDposed to communicate the department’s philosophy, mission statement, goals, 125 policies, and strategies to officers. They can provide leadership as to what activities are encouraged within the department, as well as the activities that are discouraged. Leadership is considered by some scholars to be especially important in the community policing era, as leaders must effectively convey what community policing is, how officers should practice it, and how the organization will provide the necessary support to accomplish it. Leaders can also convey values and beliefs that they feel will increase efficiency and productivity within a community policing context. In one study that used social capital as a framework for understanding community policing partnerships, the failure of the community policing program was attributed in part to, “a lack of proper leadership in the police department to promote and enforce norms of trust, reciprocity, and co-production” (Pino, 2001, p. 213). Chiefs in both Indianapolis and St. Petersburg were hired due to their support and promotion of a community policing philosophy, but they varied in how they translated this philosophy into practice (see DeJong, Mastrofski, & Parks, 2001; Parks, et al. 1999). In other words, the “vision” of community policing is substantially different for the two Clniefs. In Indianapolis, the chief encouraged officers to engage in community policing Vi a an aggressive order maintenance response. In other words, the leadership he p1‘<)vided facilitated an increase use of traditional police tactics (e.g., stops, arrests, Seaqches and seizures) in an attempt to increase residents’ feelings of safety. The up Mership” element of community policing was accomplished primarily at the district 13"” (:1, with staff members attending community meetings. Officer-level engagement of th% community was not encouraged. The small proportion of officers with specific c0Immunity policing assignments were known as “Crime Bill” officers; they were 126 supposed to work together on community policing projects. Collaboration with community groups or patrol officers was not emphasized. In short, community policing efforts in Indianapolis were “compartmentalized” as the responsibility of a few organizational members. Alternatively, the style of community policing exhorted by the chief in St. Petersburg focused on problem-solving. In fact, he had gained an international reputation for the geographic deployment of officers to enhance their ability to engage the community. In contrast to Indianapolis, community partnerships were encouraged at the officer-level rather than at the district-level. Community policing officers were supposed to work with patrol officers as a team to problem-solve in their assigned areas. The chief emphasized that community policing was a responsibility of all the officers in the department, not just those with special community policing assignments. In short, community policing efforts in St. Petersburg were integrated into the responsibilities of all organizational members. In addition, the Chief changed the performance appraisals of all officers to reflect the new emphasis on community policing. It should be noted that there are limits to what leadership can accomplish. A nation-wide survey of police administrators found that 98% agreed that community p0 licing was a worthwhile reform effort, but 47% admitted that what community policing ac: t‘ually meant in practical terms was not clear (Wycoff, 1994). Perhaps most troubling, on ly 27% felt that implementing community policing would require extensive OI‘ganizational change, for example to policies, goals or training. Under these circumstances, Mastrofski (1998) cautions that “police agency leadership is not a driving f0 rce” for accomplishing organizational change; rather, successfirl long-term change in 127 policing usually results from leaders recognizing and “riding the wave” of broader demographic, economic, social, and technological forces (p. 183). Empirical research using data fiom the POPN also reveals the limits of leadership. DeJong, Matrofski, and Parks (2001) concluded that leadership does not play an important role in implementing new programs because officers’ belief systems (i.e., their acceptance of the community policing philosophy) were not related to the amount of time they spent on problem—solving activities. To increase the amount of time spent on problem-solving activities, the authors recommend assigning officers to special units that emphasize this type of activity and where there is time to engage in these activities. Their study is consistent with much police research finding that situational or organizational factors are much more relevant determinants of officer behavior than are attitudes or beliefs. As Trojanowicz et al. (1998) note, “administrators may expect only a limited amount of problem solving to occur by decree” (p. 188). Leadership must be coupled with the structural changes needed to support officer engagement in community policing. Some of these relevant changes are discussed below. _Organizational Structure Geogaphic Responsibilig. “For community policing to be successful there must be some level of geographic permanence” (Troj anowicz, Kappeler, & Gaines, 2002, p. I 3). Geographic permanence promotes ownership and responsibility among police for wh at happens “on their beat.” The community policing philosophy dictates that officers ShQ ‘uld be integrated into the community, and this is best achieved by having them pe31‘1-nanently assigned to a particular area. In St. Petersburg, community policing officers W'erked with the general patrol officers in their assigned beats. The combined strategy of 128 geographic permanence and having all officers work together might be one explanation as to why community policing performance was more likely to occur in SPD. On the other hand, in Indianapolis, community policing officers were supposed to work together to accomplish community policing goals. In effect, this meant that their geographic responsibility covered the entire city. Goldstein’s (1990) sums up the limitation of such a strategy with his statement that “... so much of policing consists of dealing with problems. And while some problems can be viewed as citywide and relatively uniform wherever they occur, most have a local character to them or may even be unique to a specific beat. It requires officers close to a community to identify them and to deal with them” (p. 160). Coupled with the fact that officers in Indianapolis were not encouraged to work with the general patrol officers assigned to particular areas, it is not surprising that their levels of community policing performance were lower than in St. Petersburg. Decentralization. This strategy assumes that community policing will be best accomplished when officers work in an organization that is not controlled centrally, but rather decentralized to enable variation in policing styles and strategies based on the Characteristics and needs of different neighborhoods within a department’s jurisdiction. D epartments serious about community policing therefore push responsibility and au tlrority down the organizational hierarchy rather than keeping it at headquarters. This re 8 tructuring is expected to enhance officer effectiveness because they are freed from rigi d, standardized operating procedures and given the flexibility to create custom plans to ='=‘I.ddress specific problems in their assigned beats. Decentralization empowers officers to use their discretion creatively without having their activities dictated to them by upper- 129 management. Attempts to flatten the organizational structure to facilitate community policing took different forms in the two departments. In Indianapolis, community policing tasks were decentralized to the district level. This meant that district commanders were responsible for setting community policing goals and tasks and overseeing community policing projects occurring within their districts. To a certain extent then, officers still had their activities and priorities set for them by a member of management. St. Petersburg more fully realized decentralization because community policing was decentralized to the officer level. This meant that individual officers would implement and develop community policing projects with the citizens they encountered on a daily basis. They were trusted to use their discretion appropriately to determine the types of community policing activities in which to engage. In terms of designing an organizational structure that facilitated officers engaging in community policing, therefore, SPD was more successfirl than IPD. Methodological Issues Several methodological issues were raised by this research, including the inelusion of interaction terms in the study of social capital, the revelation that different regression equations are needed for modeling community policing time and community po 1 icing acts, the problem of establishing causality, the sampling strategy, and the “1% asurement of social capital and community policing. They are discussed in the 83% tions below. t eraction Terms \— Interaction terms are an important area for future researchers to explore. While 130 the present study did not reveal any significant interaction effects, it should be noted that this was more than likely due to the multicollinearity present in most of the interaction models. Recall that multicollinearity does not bias the regression results, but makes finding significant effects more difficult because the confidence intervals are increased and the t-statistics tend to be small. Significant interaction effects in the data, therefore, could have been masked by multicollinearity. Given that the interaction models tested in the current study were structured according to theoretical considerations, future researchers may want to test social capital interaction terms on larger sample sizes. This may enable enough statistical power to reveal significant effects. Friedrich (1982) reminds us that there are many beneficial reasons for testing interaction effects. Most importantly, they are more accurate and detailed descriptions of relationships that exist in social science data. In other words, including interaction effects allows the researcher avoid falling into the trap of oversirnplifying what is by all accounts an extremely complex social reality. Assuming additive effects is the most common oversirnplification of the social world. Instances of an independent variable always having the same effect on a dependent variable, regardless of the levels of the other independent variables, are surely more rare than instances where these relationships are conditional. As Friedrich (1982) notes, conditional relationships may be “less than wholly satisfying” given our predisposition to favor simple, consistent relationships, but they are “often an accurate reflection of social reality” (p. 832). Interactive models are more likely to represent the effect of social capital on any outcome under investigation, and should be included in future studies of both social capital and community policing. 131 Regession Equations That the Zero-Inflated Negative Binomial model provided the best fit to the community policing time models indicates that a different process is occurring for those officers who spend no time on community policing compared to those officers that spend some time on community policing. In contrast, no such distinction occurs for the community policing acts models, as the fit of these models to the data did not improve from zero alteration. How can this difference be explained? Recall that community policing time was derived from activity-level data, including problem-solving, crime prevention, and attending community meetings. These activities are under the purview of individual officers; they could choose whether and when to engage in them. In this way they can be considered proactive activities, and evidently many officers used their discretion to avoid engaging in them. Community policing acts (providing comfort, referrals, or information), however, require the presence of a citizen. This implies that officers were responding to calls for assistance, or engaging in some form of reactive policing. Community policing acts could be considered less under officer control compared to community policing time. This is a fundamental difference between the two dependent measures that could explain why the different regression equations were necessary. Officers were ultimately able to choose whether to spend time on community policing, whereas providing community policing to citizens was at least in part a function of whether citizens were present and needing assistance. Future research on community policing should take into account that different equations are needed to model different conceptualizations of community policing. 132 Causal Order The major problem with cross-sectional research, such as the current study, is that the researcher is unable to definitively specify the temporal order of variables, which is a major barrier to drawing causal inferences. Many researchers use theory or intuition to specify which variables are likely to come first, or predict the dependent variable in a regression equation. But these specifications may be faulty, or at least open to debate if they are tested using cross-sectional data. In the field of social capital, for example, researchers have expressed a concern related to social capital and how to “separate what it is from what it does” (Edwards & Foley, 1997, p. 669) because “equating social capital with the resources acquired through it can easily lead to tautological statements” (Portes, 1998, p. 5). The model tested in this research conceptualized social capital as a predictor of community policing, the idea being that officers with high levels of social capital would be more productive than officers who did not have this resource to draw upon to “get things done.” In light of the findings, however, it might be productive to reconsider this conceptualization. The relationship between social capital and community policing, if one exists, may be more complex. For example, a feedback-loop arrangement could exist where social capital and community policing are mutually reinforcing. That is, an increase in one leads to an increase in the other, and vice versa, and the cycle continues. The literature on social capital and educational outcomes has demonstrated that this is a distinct possibility. Alternatively, we might conceptualize the relationship as having the reverse causal order: community policing performance could be influencing levels of police social capital. This model is feasible if we consider the possibility that productivity may 133 increase social capital. For example, it makes sense to think of officers who frequently engage in problem-solving, crime prevention, or attending community meetings as being able to increase their social capital because they are involved in projects that put them into contact with people with whom they may eventually form quality relationships. They may also be forced to share information and cooperate with other officers in order to successfully complete many community policing projects; this could also increase their social capital. Similarly, officers who frequently provide comfort, referrals, and information to citizens may be expected to have higher levels of social capital compared to officers who do not frequently engage the citizenry. These examples suggest that future researchers may want to carefully consider the causal order of the social capital and outcome constructs. However, it must be restated that the body of literature consulted for this research suggested that it was entirely appropriate to conceptualize social capital as a predictor, rather than an outcome, of community policing. Sampling Strategy One significant limitation of the present study was that it relied on secondary analysis of data that were collected using a sampling strategy based on rides instead of officers. Consequently officers included in the sample had varying levels of observation — some officers were observed for many rides while others were only observed once. Officers who were not observed at all (but who participated in the interviews) were excluded from the present study. Future researchers would be advised to carefully consider the implications of having a sampling fiamework that is not consistent with the goal of the study or the unit of analysis. The present study may have yielded different results had the observational data been collected according to an officer-level sampling 134 strategy, ideally with each officer being observed multiple times over an extended period (say, six months or one year). This would be a much more effective way for gather information about specific officers and the relative impact of social capital on their community policing performance. Measurement of Communig Policing Community policing time per shift and acts per citizen represent less-than-perfect measures of community policing performance. While the goal was to create comprehensive measures that included all the activities relevant to community-oriented policing, it cannot be assumed that every community-oriented activity is captured by the data. Despite this limitation, the dependent variables in this study included a broad set of activities, all of which are guided by the community policing philosophy, representing the themes of community engagement, problem-solving, and providing assistance to citizens. The use of two different indicators (one based on time, one based on acts per citizen) also advances the study of community policing as it draws attention to the different ways that the philosophy is translated into practice. Future researchers may want to consider the effect of combining different community policing indicators into global measures of community policing. It might be more instructive to model the impact of social capital on each of the community policing tasks separately to avoid masking different relationships across these tasks. For example, it is reasonable to suppose that police social capital might matter more to officers engaged in problem-solving efforts than to officers attending community meetings. The former is often a group activity where personal relationships could make a difference whereas the latter is usually conducted at the officer’s discretion. Similarly, police social 135 capital could impact the proclivity of officers to provide information or referrals to citizens as they may draw upon their work relationships for information relevant to citizen needs. Providing comfort, however, is more than likely the result of either necessity or an individual officer’s belief that comforting citizens is important rather than the quality officers’ relationships with their peers or supervisors. Modeling these activities separately could shed light on the different ways that social capital might impact officer performance. Measurement of Social Capital The current study makes several improvements in the measurement of the social capital construct. While the social capital research has been plagued by inadequate operationalization of this complex construct, the measures used in the present study are both more specific and more comprehensive than those previously used in social capital research. Four social capital dimensions are specified: level of trust, cooperative exchanges, group cohesion, and social support. Previous social capital research typically uses only one or two variables representing one part of what has come to be known as “social capital,” whereas this study advances the notion that this construct is multidimensional, and uses multiple items to measure these dimensions. Despite the advances the current study makes over previous social capital research, there are limitations to the measures of social capital. The one that most clearly stands out is the variable used to measure trust. Ideally several measures would be used to tap into thisidimension. In addition, it would be beneficial to have questions assessing officers’ trust for each person in their work unit, as well as their supervisors. The same can be said for the social support dimension. Although a reliable factor, it only reveals 136 the level of support an officer receives from his or her supervisor. The level of support an officer perceives having from his or her peer group would also be an important element of social capital that is missing fiom this measure. The other two social capital dimensions (cooperative exchanges and group cohesion) have multiple measures that assess these areas in terms of both the officers’ peers and supervisors, but neither scale reached the conventional standard of reliability. Given the limitations of these key theoretical variables, the lack of significant findings is not surprising. Future research on police social capital should attempt to incorporate information on both the quantity and quality of both peer and supervisor relationships. An in—depth examination into one specific work group, including both qualitative and quantitative data collection, would aid our understanding of how and why police social capital is related to the performance of various policing activities. Officers could be asked specific questions about their peer and supervisor relationships, how these relationships help or hinder their performance, and their perceptions of the dimensions in these relationships that constitute the most important source of social capital (trust, cooperation, cohesion, or support). Additionally, observations of police-peer and police-supervisor interactions would constitute a valuable source of data that would reveal the formation and utilization of police social capital within this unique work environment. 137 APPENDIX A Table 14 Correlation Matrix of All Variables. CP Time CP Acts Female Non-White Educ. Tenure CP Time 1 -0.04 0.07 -0.05 -0.07 -0.02 CP Acts -0.04 1 0.07 -0.05 0.07 —0.17*** Female 0.07 0.07 l 0.15** 0.03 0.02 Non-White -0.05 -0.05 0.15** 1 0.06 -0.07 Education -0.07 0.07 -0.03 0.06 1 -0.31*** Tenure -0.02 -0. l 7*** 0.02 -0.07 -0.31*** l CPO 0.11* 0.06 -0.07 0.02 -0.05 0.04 CP Training 0.05 -0.08 0.11* 0.20*** -0.03 -0.28*** IPD -0.1 -0.22*** 0.09 0.03 0.11* -0.04 Day Shift 0.01 -0.10* 0.04 0.08 -0.12** 0.42*** Beat Prob. -0.09 -0.05 0.18*** 0.06 0.16** -0.21*** Dept. Pro-CP 0.00 —0.07 0.17** 0.04 -0.09 -0.05 Trust 0.05 0.02 0.06 0 -0.11* -0.05 Cooperation -0.01 -0.07 0.01 -0.07 -0.03 -0.14** Cohesion 0.00 -0.04 -0.05 —0.1 1* -0. 14** -0.02 Support 0.00 0.05 0.04 -0.03 -0.12* -0.05 138 Table 14 (cont’d). CPO CP IPD Day Beat Training Shift Problems CP Time 0.1 1* 0.05 -0.1 0.01 -0.09 CP Acts 0.06 -0.08 -0.22*** -0.1* -0.05 Female 007 0.11* 0.09 0.04 0.18*** N on-White 0.02 0.20*** 0.03 0.08 0.06 Education -0.05 -0.03 0.11* -0.12* 0.16** Tenure 0.04 -0.28*** -0.04 0.42*** -0.21*** CPO 1 -0.40*** -0.72*** 0.32*** -0.21*** CP Training -0.40*** l 0.39*** -0.26*** 0.2*** IPD -0.72*** 0.39*** l -0.08 0.33*** Day Shift 0.32*** -0.26*** -0.08 1 -0.04 Beat Prob. -0.21*** 0.20*** 0.33*** -0.04 1 Dept. Pro-CP —0.40*** 0.43*** 0.31 *** —0.15** 0.05 Trust 0.05 0.1 -0.09 -0.02 -0.16** Cooperation -0. 12* 0.24*** 0.08 -0.1 1* 0. 14* Cohesion -0.23*** 0.25*** 0.07 —0.21*** -0.01 Support 0.04 0.06 -0.06 -0.04 -O.l3** 139 Table 14 (cont’d). Dept. Trust Cooperation Cohesion Support Pro-CP CP Time 0 0.05 -0.01 0 0 CP Acts -0.07 0.02 -0.07 -0.04 0.05 Female 0.17** 0.06 0.01 -0.05 0.04 Non-White 0.04 0 -0.07 -0.1 1* -0.03 Education -0.09 -0.1 1* -0.03 -0.14** -0.l2* Tenure -0.05 -0.05 -0.14** -0.02 -0.05 CPO -0.4*** 0.05 -0.12* -0.23*** 0.04 CP Training 0.43*** 0.1 0.24*** 0.25*** 0.06 IPD 0.31 *** -0.09 0.08 0.07 -0.06 Day Shift -0.15** -0.02 -0.11* -0.21*** -0.04 Beat Prob. 0.05 -O.16** 0.14** -0.01 -0.13** Dept. Pro-CP 1 0.27*** 0.23*** 0.32*** 0.25*** Trust 0.27*** 1 0.25*** 0.35*** 0.83*** Cooperation 0.23*** 0.25*** 1 0.33*** 0.26*** Cohesion 0.32*** 0.35*** 0.33*** 1 0.35*** Support 0.25*** 0.83*** 0.26*** 0.35*** 1 N=262 Notes: * (p < .10) ** (p < .05) *** (p < .01) 140 REFERENCES Aasland, O. G., Ekeberg, O., & Schweder, T. (2001). Suicide rates fiom 1960 to 1989 in Norwegian physicians compared with other educational groups. Social Science and Medicine, 52(2), 259-265. Allison, P. D., & Long, J. S. (1990). Departmental effects on scientific productivity. American Soci01001ca1 Review 55 469-478. Alpert, G. P., & Moore, M. H. (1998). Measuring police performance in the new paradigm of policing. In G. P. Alpert & A. Piquero (Eds), Community Policing: Contemporary Readings (pp. 215-232). Prospect Heights, IL: Waveland. Antonucci, T. C. (1994). A life-span view of women’s social relations. In B. F. Turner & L. W. Troll (Eds), Women Growing Older (pp. 239-269). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Antonucci, T. C., Akiyarna, H., and Lansford, J. E. (1998). Negative effects of close social relations. Family Relations, 47, 379-384. Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51(6 ), 1173-1182. Bayley, D. H. (1986). The tactical choices of police patrol officers. Journal of Criminal Justice 14 329-348. Bayley, D. H. (1994). Police for the future. New York: Oxford University Press. Bayley, D. H., & Bittner, E. (1993). Learning the skills of policing. In Dunham, R. G., & Alpert, G. P. (Eds.) Critical Issues in Policing (pp. 106-129). Prospect Heights, IL: Waveland. Beck, K., & Wilson, C. (1997). Police officers’ views on cultivating organizational commitment: Implications for police managers. Policing: An International Journg of Police Strategies and Management, 20, 175-195. Bittner, E. (1970). The functions of police in modern society. Rockville: NIMH. Bittner, E. (1983). Legality and workmanship: Introduction to control in the police organization. In M. Punch (Ed.), Control in the Police Organization (pp. 1-11). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. 141 Bittner, E. (1990). Aspects of police work. Boston: Northeastern University Press. Bloch, P., & Anderson, D. (1974). Policewomen on patrol: Final report. Washington, DC: Urban Institute. Bollen, K. A., & Long, J. S. (1993). Testing structural equation models. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Bourdieu, P. (1984). Distinction: A social critique of the judgment of taste. Cambridge, England: Harvard University Press. Bourdieu, P. (1986). The forms of capital. In J. G. Richardson (Ed.), Handbook of Theog and Research for the Sociology of Education (pp. 241-258). New York: Greenwood Press. Bourdieu, P. (199). The logic of practice. Cambridge, UK: Polity. Brehm, J ., & Rahn, W. (1997). Individual-level evidence for the causes and consequences of social capital. American Journal of Political Science. 41. 999-1023. Bryk, A. S. & Raudenbush, S. W. (1992). Hierarchical linear models. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Bursick, R. J ., Jr. (1999). The informal control of crime through neighborhood networks. Sociological Focus, 32, 85-97. Burt, R. S. (1997). The contingent value of social capital. Administrative Science QuarterlYJZ. 339-365. Buzawa, E. S. (1981). The role of race in predicting job attitudes of patrol officers. Journal of Criminal Justice 9, 63-77. Caldwell, E. ( 1978). Patrol observation: The patrol encounter, patrol narrative, and general shift information forms. Police Services Study Methods Report Mr-2. Bloomington, IN: Indiana University. Cameron, A. C., & Trivedi, P. K. (1986). Econometric models based on count data: Comparisons and applications of some estimators and tests. J oumal of Applied Econometrics, L 29-53. Carmines, E. G., & Zeller, R. A. (1979). Reliability and validitv assesament. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. 142 Carter, D. L. (1986). Hispanic perception of police performance: An empirical assessment. Journal of Criminal Justice. 13. 487-500. Coleman, J. S. (1988). Social capital in the creation of human capital. American Journal of Sociology. 94. 895-8120. Cooper, R. K. (1986). Occupational stress in police work (Doctoral dissertation, University of Minnesota). Ann Arbor, MI: University Microfilrns International. Cordner, G. W. (1998). Community policing: Elements and effects. In G. P. Alpert & A. Piquero (Eds), Communig Policing: Contemporg Readings (pp. 45-62). Prospect Heights, IL: Waveland. Crank, J. P. (1998). Understanding police culture. Cincinnati, OH: Anderson. Cutrona, C. E. (1996). Social support in couples. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. DeJong, C. (2000). Gender differences in officer attitude and behavior: Providing comfort to citizens. Manuscript submitted for publication, Michigan State University. DeJong, C., Mastrofski, S. D., & Parks, R. B. (2001). Patrol officers and problem solving: An application of expectancy theory. Justice Quarterly. 18. 31-62. Duffee, D. E., Fluellen, R., & Roscoe, T. (1999). Constituency building and urban commrmity policing. In R. H. Langworthy (Ed.), Measuring what matters: Proceedings from the policing research institute meetings (pp. 91-119). Washington, DC: National Institute of Justice. D’Unger, A. V., Land, K. C., McCall, P. L., & Nagin, D. S. (1998). How many latent classes of delinquent/criminal careers? Results from mixed Poisson regression analyses. American Journal of Sociology, 103(6). 1593-1630. Eck, J. E. & Spelman, W. (1987). Who ya gonna call? The police as problem- busters. Crime and Delinquency, 33, 31-52. Edwards, B., & Foley, M. W. (1997). Social capital and the political economy of our discontent. American Behaviofiral Scientist, 40. 669-678. Ellison,-K. W., & Genz, J. (1983). StreSflnd the police officer. Springfield, IL: Thomas. Feinman, C. (1994). Women in the criminal iustice mtem. Westport, CT: Praeger. 143 Foley, M. W., & Edwards, B. (1997). Escape from politics? Social theory and the social capital debate. American Behavioral Scientist. 40. 550-561. Frank, K. A., & Yasumoto, J. Y. (1998). Linking action to social structure within a system: Social capital within and between subgroups. American Journal of Sociology. 104, 642-86. Friedrich, R. J. (1982). In defense of multiplicative terms in multiple regression equations. American Journal of Political Science. 26(4). 797-833. Furstenberg, F. F., & Hughes, M. E. (1995). Social capital and successful development among at-risk youth. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 57, 580-592. F urstenberg, F. F ., Jr., & Wellford, C. F. (1973). Calling the police: The evaluation of police service. Law and SocietyReview. 7. 393-406. Fyfe, J. J. (1981). Who shoots? A look at officer race and police shooting. Journal of Police Science and Administration 9, 367-382. F yfe, J. J. (1999). Police use of deadly force: Research and reform. In Gaines, L. K., & Cordner, G. W. (Eds), Policing Perspectives: An Anthology (pp. 408-434). Los Angeles: Roxbury. Geller, W. A., & Swanger, G. (1995). Managing innovation in policing: The untapped potential of the middle manager. Washington, DC: Police Executive Research Forum. Glensor, R. W., & Peak, K. J. (2000). Implementing change: Community-oriented policing and problem-solving. In R. W. Glensor, M. E. Correia, & K. J. Peak (Eds), Policing Communities: Understanding Crime and Solving Problems (pp. 239-245). Los Angeles: Roxbury. Goldstein, H. (1977). Policing a free society. Cambridge, MA: Ballinger. Goldstein, H. (1990). Problem-oriented policing. New York: McGraw-Hill. Goldstein, H. (1998). Toward community-oriented policing: Potential, basic requirements, and threshold questions. In Alpert, G. P., & Piquero, A. (Eds), Community Policing: Contemporary Readings (pp. 3-22). Prospect Heights, IL: Waveland. Granovetter, M. S. (1973). The strength of weak ties. American Jouma_qf Sociology, 78, 1360-1380. 144 Greene, J. R. (1998). Evaluating planned change strategies in modern law enforcement: Implementing community-based policing. In J -P. Brodeur (Ed.), How to recogpize good policing (pp. 141-160). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Greene, J. R., & Klockars, C. B. (1991). What police do. In Klockars, C. B., & Mastroflcsi, S. D. (Eds), Thinking About Police: Contemporarv Readings (pp. 273-284). Boston: McGraw-Hill. Greene, W. H. (2000). Econometric analysis (4th Edition). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Grogger, J. (1990). The deterrent effect of capital punishment: An analysis of daily homicide counts. Journal of the American Statistical Association. 85. 295-303. Gulati, R. (1995). Social structure and alliance formation patterns: A longitudinal analysis. Administrative Science Quarterly. 40. 619-652. Haarr, R. N. (1997). Patterns of interaction in a police patrol bureau: Race and gender barriers to integration. Justice Quarterly, 14, 53-85. Hagan, F. E. (1993). Research methods in criminal justice and criminology. New York: Macmillan. Hale, D. C., & Wyland, S. M. (1999). Dragons and dinosaurs: The plight of patrol women. In Gaines, L. K., & Cordner, G. W. (Eds), Policing Perapectives: An Anthology (pp. 450-458). Los Angeles: Roxbury. Hemstrom, O. (1999). Does the work environment contribute to excess male mortality? Social Science and Medicine. 49(7). 879-894. Hoath, D. R., Schneider, F. W., & Starr, M. W. (1998). Police job satisfaction as a function of career orientation and position tenure: Implications for selection and community policing. Journal of Criminal Justice 26, 337-347. Hofferth, S. L., & Iceland, J. (1998). Social capital in rural and urban communities. Rural Sociology, 63, 574-598. Holdaway, S., & Barron, A. M. (1997). Resigers? The experience of Black and Asian police officers. London: Macmillan. Hope, T. (1998). Privatopia on trial? Property guardianship in the suburbs. In K. Painter & N. T illey (Eds), Surveillance (special issue, Crime Prevention Studies, forthcoming). Morsey, NY: Criminal Justice Press. 145 J accard, J ., Tunisi, R., & Wan, C. K. (1990). Interaction effects in multiple regession. Sage Series in Quantitative Applications in the Social Sciences, volume 72. Jacobs, D., & O’Brien, R. M. (1998). The determinants of deadly force: A structural analysis of police violence. American Journal of Sociology, 103(4), 837-862. J 01in, A., & Moose, C. A. (1997). Evaluating a domestic violence program in a community policing environment: Research implementation issues. Crime and DeliquencyL43. 279-97. Kakar, S. (1998). Self-evaluations of police performance: An analysis of the relationship between police officers’ education level and job performance. Policing: An International Journal of Police Straiegies and Management, 21, 632-47. Kappeler, V. E., Carter, D., & Sapp, A. (1992). Police officer higher education, citizen complaints, and departmental rule violations. American Journal of Police. 11. 37- 54. Kawachi, 1., Kennedy, B. P., & Wilkinson, R G. (1999). Crime: social disorganization and relative deprivation. Social Science and Medicine. 48. 719-731. Kelling, G. (1999). Measuring what matters: A new way of thinking about crime and public order. In R. H. Langworthy (Ed.), Measuring What Matters: Proceedings From the Policing Research Institute Meetings (pp. 27-35). Washington, DC: National Institute of Justice. Kelling, G. L. & Moore, M. (1988). The Evolving Strategy of Policing. Perspective on Policing (No. 4). Washington, DC: National Institute of Justice. Kennedy, B. P., Kawachi, I., Prothrow-Stith, D., Lochner, K., & Gupta, V. (1998). Social capital, income inequality, and firearm violent crime. Social Science an_d Medicine. 47. 7-17. Klinger, D. A. (1997). Negotiating order in patrol work: An ecological theory of police response to deviance. Criminology, 35, 277-306. Land, K. C., McCall, P. L., & Nagin, D. S. (1996). A comparison of Poisson, negative binomial, and semipararnetric mixed Poisson regression models with empirical applications to criminal careers data. Sociological Methods and Research, 24(4), 387- 442. Lewis, S., Rosenberg, H., & Sigler, R. T. (1999). Acceptance of community policing among police officers and police administrators. Policing: An International 146 J ournral of Police Strategies and Management, 22, 567-588. Long, J. S. (1997). Regression models for categorical and limited dependent variables. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Lurigio, A. J ., & Rosenbaum, D. P. (1994). The impact of community policing on police personnel: A review of the literature. In Rosenbaum, D. P. (Ed.), The Challenge of Communiiy Policing: Testing the Promises (pp. 147-63). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Lurigio, A. J ., & Skogan, W. G. (2000). Winning the hearts and minds of police officers: An assessment of staff perceptions of community policing in Chicago. In R. W. Glensor, M. E. Correia, & K. J. Peak (Eds), Policing Communities: Understanding Crime and Solving Problems (pp. 246-256). Los Angeles: Roxbury. Lyons, W. (1999). The politics of community policing: Rearranging the power to punish. Ann Arbor, MI: The University of Michigan Press. Mahar, C., Harker, R., & Wilkes, C. (1990). The basic theoretical position. In R. Harker, C. Mahar, & C. Wilkes (Eds) An introduction to the work of Pierre Bourdieu: Thmctice of theory (pp. 1-25). London: Macmillan. Manning, P. K. (1994). The police: Symbolic capital, class, and control. In G. S. Bridges & M. A. Myers (Eds), Inequalig, Crime, and Social Control (pp. 80-96). Oxford: Westview. Manning, P. K. (1997). Police work (2nd edition). Prospect Heights, IL: Waveland. Manning, P. K., & Van Maanen, J. (1978). Socialization for policing. In P. K. Manning & J. Van Maanen (Eds). Policirg: A View from the Street (pp.267-272). Santa Monica, CA: Goodyear. Martin, S. E. (1980). Breaking and entering: Policewomen on patrol. Berkeley, CA: The University of California Press. Martin, S. E. (1993). Female officers on the move? A status report on women in policing. In Dunham, R. G., & Alpert, G. P. (Eds), Critical Issues in Policing (pp. 327 - 347). Prospect Heights, IL: Waveland. Martin, S. E. (1994). ‘Outsider within’ the station house: The impact of race and gender on black women police. Social Problems 41, 383-400. Martin, S. E., & Jurik, N. C. (1996). Doing justice, doing gender. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. 147 Marx, G. T. (1978). Alternative measures of police performance. In R. C. Larson (Ed), Police Accorflzfirility (pp. 15-32). Lexington, MA: Lexington Books. Mastrofski, S. D. (1981). Surveying clients to assess police performance: Focusing on the police-citizen encounter. Evaluation Review, 5, 397-408. Mastrofski, S. D. (1982). Issues in police performance measurement. Urban Interest 4, 3-6. Mastrofski, S. D. (1996). Measuring police performance in public encounters. In L. T. Hoover (Ed.), uanti 'n uali in Policin (pp. 207-241). Washington, DC: Police Executive Research Forum. Mastrofski, S. D. (1999). Policing for people. Washington, DC: Police Foundation. Mastrofski, S. D., Snipes, J. B., Parks, R. B., & Maxwell, C. D. (2000). The helping hand of the law: Police control of citizens on request. Criminology, 38, 307-342. Mastrofski, S. D., Worden, R. E., & Snipes, J. B. (1995). Law enforcement in a time of community policing. Criminology, 33, 539-563. McCarthy, B., & Hagan, J. (1995). Getting into street crime: The structure and process of criminal embeddedness. Social Science Research. 24. 63-95. Miller, S. L. (1998). Rocking the rank-and-file: gender issues and community policing. Journal of Contemporaiy Criminal Justice, 14, 156-172. Miller, S. L. (1999). Gender and communim policing: Walking the talk. Boston: Northeastern University Press. Milutinovich, J. S. (1977). Black-white differences in job satisfaction, group cohesiveness, and leadership style. Human Relations 30, 1079-87. Molinas, J. R. (1998). The impact of inequality, gender, external assistance and social capital on local-level cooperation. World Development, 26, 413-431. Moore, G. (1990). Structural determinants of men’s and women’s personal networks. American Sociological Review. 55. 726-735. Moore, M. H. (1992). Problem solving and community policing. In M. Tonry & N. Morris (Eds), Modern Policing (pp. 99-158). Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Morash, M., & Greene, J. R. (1986). Evaluating women on patrol: A critique of 148 contemporary wisdom. Evaluation Review. 10. 230-255. Morash, M., & Haarr, R. N. (1995). Gender, workplace problems, and stress in policing. Justice Quarterly, 12, 113-140. Morrell, S., Taylor, R., & Slaytor, E. (1999). Urban and rural suicide differentials in migrants and the Australian-bom, New South Wales, Australia 1985-1994. Social Science and Medicine, 49( 1). 81-91. Muir, W. K., Jr. (1977). Police: Streetcomer politicians. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press. Nagin, D. S., & Land, K. C. (1993). Age, criminal careers, and population heterogeneity: Specification and estimation of a nonparametric, mixed Poisson model. Criminology, 31(3 ), 327-362. Nelson, J. F. (1980). Multiple victimization in American cities: A statistical analysis of rare events. American Journal of Sociology. 85(4). 870-891. Newton, K. (1997). Social capital and democracy. American Behavioral Scientist @, 575-586. Niederhoffer, A. (1967). Behind the shield: The police in urban sociegi. Garden City, NY: Doubleday. Osborne, D. R., & Tseloni, A. (1998). The distribution of household property crimes. Journal of Quantitative Criminology, 14(3), 307-330. Palombo, B. J. (1995). Academic professionalism in law enforcement. New York: Garland. Paoline, E. A., Myers, S. M., & Worden, R. E. (2000). Police culture, individualism, and community policing: Evidence from two police departments. Justice Quarterly, 17(3 ), 575-605. Parks, R. B., Mastrofski, S. D., DeJong, C., and Gray, M. K. (1999). How officers spend their time with the community. Justice Quarterly, 16(3), 483-518. Pate, A. M., & Shtull, P. (1994). Community policing grows in Brooklyn: An inside view of the New York City police department’s model precinct. Crime and Delinquency, 40, 384-410. Peillon, M. (1998). Bourdieu’s field and the sociology of welfare. Journal of Social Policv. 27. 213-29. Pino, N. W. (2001). Community policing and social capital. Policing: An 149 Interaational J ourng of Police Strategieaand Management, 24(2 ), 200-215. Portes, A. (1998). Social capital: Its origins and applications in modern sociology. Annual Review of Sociology. 24. 1-24. Portes, A., & Landolt, P. (1996). The downside of social capital. The American Prospect. 26. 18-21, 94. Portney, K. E., & Berry, J. M. (1997). Mobilizing minority communities: Social capital and participation in urban neighborhoods. American Beliayioral ScientistAO. 632-344. Reiner, R. (1998). Process or product? Problems of assessing individual police performance. In J -P. Brodeur (Ed.), How to recogpize good policing (pp. 55-72). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Reiss, A. J ., Jr. (1968). Police brutality — annswers to key questions. Transaction, 5, 10-19. Reuss-Ianni, E. (1983). Two cultures of policing: Street cops and management cops. New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction. Roberg, R., Crank, J ., & Kuykendall, J. (2000). Police and society. Los Angeles: Roxbury. Robinson, A. L., & Chandek, M. S. (20003). Philosophy into practice? Community policing units and domestic violence victim participation. Policing: An International Journal of Police Strategies and Management, 23(3 ), 280-302. Robinson, A. L., & Chandek, M. S. (2000b). The Domestic Violence Arrest Decision: Examining Demographic, Attitudinal, and Situational Variables. Crime and Delinquency, 46(1), 18-37. Robinson, A. L., & Morash, M. (2000). Use of the social capital fi'amework for assessment of a training and education program for people working with crime victims. The Justice Professional 13(4), 307-341. Rose, D. R., & Clear, T. R. (1999). Incarceration, social capital, and crime: Implications for social disorganization theory. Criminology, 36, 441-479. Rosenbaum, D. P. (1998). The changing role of the police: Assessing the current transition to community policing. In J-P. Brodeur (Ed.), How to recogize good policing (pp. 3-29). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Rosenbaum, D. P., & Lurigio, A. J. (1994). An inside look at community policing 150 reform: Definitions, organizational changes, and evaluation findings. Crime and Delinquency, 40, 299-314. Rountree, P. W., & Warner, B. D. (1999). Social ties and crime: Is the relationship gendered? Criminology, 37, 789-814. Rubinstein, J. (1973). Cig police. New York: Farrar, Straus, and Giroux. Sacco, V. F. (1998). Evaluating satisfaction. In J -P. Brodeur (Ed), How to recogr_1ize good policing (pp. 123-137). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Sadd, S., & Grinc, R. M. (2000). Implementation challenges in community policing: Innovative neighborhood-oriented policing in eight cities. In R. W. Glensor, M. E. Correia, & K. J. Peak (Eds), Policing Communities: Understanding Crime and Solving Problems (pp. 97-118). Los Angeles: Roxbury. Sampson, R. J. (1991). Linking the micro- and macro-level dimensions of community social organization. Social Forces. 70. 43-64. Sampson, R. J. (1995). The cormnunity. In J. Q. Wilson & J. Petersilia (Eds) Crime (pp. 193-216). San Francisco, CA: ICS Press. Sampson, R. J. (1997). The embeddedness of child and adolescent development: A community-level perspective on urban violence. In J. McCord (Ed.), Violence and Childhood in the Inner Ciiy (pp. 31-77). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Sampson, R. J ., Morenoff, J. D., & Earls, F. (1999). Beyond social capital: Spatial dynamics of collective efficacy for children. American Sociological Review. 64. 633- 660. Sampson, R. J ., Raudenbush, S. W., & Earls, F. (1997). Neighborhoods and violent crime: A multilevel study of collective efficacy. Science 277 918-24. Sampson, R. J ., & Raudenbush, S. W. (1999). Systematic social observation of public spaces: A new look at disorder in urban neighborhoods. American Journal of Sociology. 105(3). 603-651. Sampson, R., J. & Scott, M. S. (1999). Tackling crime and other public-safeiy problems: Case studies in problem-solving. Washington, DC: Department of Justice, Office of Community Oriented Policing Services. Schumacker, R. E., & Lomax, R. G. (1996). A beginner’s gpide to structural equation modeling. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. 151 Sherman, L. W. (1978). The quality of police education. (Ch. 7). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Sherman, L. W., Smith, D. A., Schmidt, J. D., & Rogan, D. P. (1992). Crime, punishment, and stake in conformity: Legal and informal control of domestic violence. American Sociological Review. 57. 680-690. Sichel, J. L., Friedman, L. N., Quint, J. C., & Smith, M. E. (1078). Women on patrol: A pilot study of police performance in New YoraCity. Washington, DC: National Institute of Law Enforcement and Criminal Justice. Skogan, W. G. (1989). Communities, crime, and neighborhood organization. Crime and Delinquency, 35, 437-457. Skogan, W. G. (1998). Community policing in Chicago. In G. P. Alpert & A. Piquero (Eds), Commqu Policing: Contemporgy Readings (pp. 159-174). Prospect Heights, IL: Waveland. Skogan, W. G. (1999). Measuring what matters: Crime, disorder, and fear. In R. H. Langworthy (Ed.), Measuring What Matters: Proceedings From the Policing Research Institute Meetings (pp. 37-53). Washington, DC: National Institute of Justice. Skogan, W. G., & Hartnett, S. M. (1997). Communig policing, Chicago style. New York: Oxford University Press. Skolnick, J. H. (1994). Justice without trial (3rd edition). New York: Macmillan. Skolnick, J. H., & Bayley, D. (1988). Communigi policing: Issues and practices around the world. Washington, DC: National Institute of Justice. Smith, D. A. (1986). The neighborhood context of police behavior. In Reiss, A. J ., Jr., & Tonry, M. (Eds), Communities and Crime (pp. 313-341). Chicago: The University of Chicago Press. Sparrow, M., Moore, M. H., & Kennedy, D. M. (1990). Beyond 911: A new era for policing. New York: Basic Books. Stalans, L. J ., & F inn, M. A. (1995). How novice and experienced officers interpret wife assaults: Normative and efficiency frames. _L_aw an_cLSociety Review. 29. 287 -32 1 . Stanton-Salazar, R. D., & Sanford, M. D. (1995). Social capital and the reproduction of inequality: Information networks among Mexican-origin high school students. Sociology of Education, 68, 116-135. 152 Stevenson, T. M. (1988). Stress among police officers: Burnout and its correlates (Doctoral dissertation, California School of Professional Psychology). Ann Arbor, MI: University Microfihns International. Teachman, J. D., Paasch, K., & Carver, K. (1997). Social capital and the generation of human capital. Social Forces. 75. 1-17. Thouez, J -P. (1984). Cancer mortality differential by social milieu: The case of the Montreal metropolitan area, 1971. Social Science and Medicine. 18(1). 73-81. Truxillo, D. M., Bennett, S. R., & Collins, M. L. (1998). College education and police job performance: A ten-year study. Public Personnel Management, 27, 269-80. Van Maanen, J. (1978). Kinsmen in repose: Occupational perspectives of patrolrnen. In P. K. Manning & J. Van Maanen (Eds), Policing: A View from the Street (pp.115-128). Santa Monica, CA: Goodyear. Van Maanen, J. (1983). The boss: First-line supervision in an American police agency. In M. Punch (Ed.), Control in the Police Organization (pp. 275-317). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Vlahov, D., Tang, A. M., & Lyles, C. (2000). Increased frequency of overdose deaths among HIV-infected injection drug users. Addiction Research. 8(4). 311-326. Waldinger, R. (1995). The ‘other side’ of embeddedness: A case-study of the interplay of economy and ethnicity. Ethnic and Racial Studies,18. 555-580. Walker, S. (1984). Broken windows and fractured history: The use and misuse of history in recent police patrol analysis. Justice Qparterly, 1, 77-90. Walker, S. (1999). The police in America (3rd edition). Boston: McGraw-Hill. Weisburd, D., & McElroy, J. E. (1988). Enacting the CPO role: Findings from the New York City pilot program in community policing. In J. R. Greene & S. D. Mastrofski (Eds), Communiiy Policing: Rhetoric or Realifl? (pp. 89-101). New York: Praeger. Weisel, D. L., & Eck, J. E. (2000). Toward a practical approach to organizational change: Community policing initiatives in six cities. In R. W. Glensor, M. E. Correia, & K. J. Peak (Eds), Policing Communities: Understanding Crime and Solving Problems (pp. 257-271). Los Angeles: Roxbury. Wellman, B., & Wortley, S. (1990). Different strokes fiom different folks: Community ties and social support. American Journal of Sociology, 96, 558-88. 153 Westley, W. A. (1970). Violence and the police. Cambridge: MIT Press. White, S. E., & Marino, K. E. (1983). Job attitudes and police stress: An exploratory study of causation. Journal of Police Science and Administration 11, 264-74. Williams, H., & Murphy, P. V. (1990). The evolving strategy of police: A minority View. No. 13 of Perspectives on Policing. Washington, DC: Government Printing Office. Williams, J., & Sloan, RC. (1990). Turning concept into practice: The Aurora Colorado story. East Lansing, MI: National Center for Community Policing. Wilson, W. J. (1987). The truly disadvantaged. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press. . Wilt, G. M., & Bannon, J. D. (1976). Cynicism or realism: A critique of Niederhoffer’s research into police attitudes. Journal of Police Science ad Administration 4, 38-45. Winfree, T. L., Jr., & Newbold, G. (1999). Community policing and the New Zealand Police: Correlates of attitudes toward the work world in a community-oriented national police organization. Policing: An International Journal of Police Strateges and Management, 22, 589-617. Wood, R. L. (1997). Social capital and political culture: God meets politics in the inner city. American Behavioral ScientisfiO. 595-605. Woolcock, M. (1998). Social capital and economic development: Toward a theoretical synthesis and policy framework. Theory and Society, 27, 151-208. Worden, R. E. (1989). Situational and attitudinal explanations of police behavior: A theoretical reappraisal and empirical assessment. Law apd Society Review. 23. 677- 711. Wycoff, M. A. (1982). Improving police performance: One more voice. Urban Interest 4, 8-16. Zhao, J ., Thurman, Q., & Lovrich, N. (2000). Communitiy-oriented policing across the US: Facilitators and Irnpediments to Implementation. In R. W. Glensor, M. E. Correia, & K. J. Peak (Eds), Policing Communities: Understanding Crime and Solving Problems (pp. 229-238). Los Angeles: Roxbury. Zorn, C. J. W. (1998). An analytic and empirical examination of zero-inflated and hurdle Poisson specifications. Sociological Methods and Research, 26(3), 368-400. 154 IIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIII 111111111(twirl 1 3 02334