, 33L. “5! .21 p . "1 :k" 35.3 at, am Ar 2... fin ... imiisaaifi 3.1.... .. s 3... {:‘l ! 1.1:...u11 .1 [12,... . .315. w . $3.5: . .1 .iv 115.1133. . I» ,4 “fem 3. 33.1.... 343... .1... iii?! .2... “1:2; :2 .I x. .5}: 0...... 2.. 5... 5:7... . .511. I. {It . L333... .3. . . 5"“. Q I! 32......43 3%. it! a . 1.. 30...”. 2. 3|! ”Dd-$1.5 7 12...: s . :J-IHWJWI 4i - ififififimfiw . fiéfigfigfigg . ThiiSiS 40° URPARY I Micrwa diam; University I This is to certify that the dissertation entitled Socioeconomic, Family, and Maternal Influences on Behavior and Verbal Ability in Young Jamaican Children presented by Elizabeth Ansel Kirsch has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for Ph . D . degree in PhilOSODhl Major professor DMe December 31, 2001 MS U i: an Affirmative Action/Equal Opportunity Institution 0-12771 PLACE IN RETURN Box to remove this checkout from your record. TO AVOID FINES return on or before date due. MAY BE RECALLED with earlier due date if requested. DATE DUE DATE DUE DATE DUE JUN 2 7 2005 'JUL—TI—ZUUS 6/01 c:/CIRClDateDue.p65~p. 15 SOCIOECONOMIC, FAMILY, AND MATERNAL INFLUENCES ON BEHAVIOR AND VERBAL ABILITY IN YOUNG JAMAICAN CHILDREN By Elizabeth Ansel Kirsch A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University In partial fulfillment of the requirements For the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Psychology 2001 ABSTRACT SOCIOECONOMIC, MATERNAL, AND FAMILY INFLUENCES ON BEHAVIOR AND VERBAL ABILITY IN YOUNG JAMAICAN CHILDREN By Elizabeth Ansel Kirsch For young children, self-regulation and the ability to use language for communication are known to make critical contributions to school success. It is also known that SES, maternal psychopathology, and family cohesion influence the development of these capacities. Virtually all evidence about these relationships, however, comes from research in Western developed countries. Parents and teachers in developing countries, of course, also want to improve early childhood education, but for economic and cultural reasons, there are limits in the extent to which this evidence can be generalized to developing countries. The goal of the current study, therefore, was to determine whether these contributors to child development are similar for Jamaica, a developing country. Jamaica is experiencing serious educational and economic problems but little is known about the development and functioning of its children and families. The current study begins to address this deficit in the knowledge base by focusing on 151 3- to 6-year-olds and their mothers. Based on the Mothers’ answers to the Brief Symptom Inventory and the Family Adaptability and cohesion Scale, each mother was rated on information on psychopaflrology, and each family was rated on family cohesion. Each mother also provided information on her family’s economic resources as well as information about her child’s behavior problems for her answers to the Connors Parent Rating Scale. Finally, each child’s verbal ability and puzzle-solving skill were assessed by the McCarthy Scales of Children’s Abilities, a measure of intelligence that includes subscales such as verbal ability and puzzle-solving. Path analysis revealed that maternal psychopathology was positively related, and family cohesion negatively related, to behavior problems. The analysis also indicated that SES was positively related to puzzle-solving, and negatively related to psychopaflrology and behavior problems. Although the results showed that Jamaica and developed countries have some paths to child behavior problems, verbal ability, and puzzle-solving in common, they also revealed several differences, thus supporting both the culture-general and culture- specific perspectives. The results thus suggest that any attempt to ameliorate child behavior problems in school or at home must take the relation between family cohesion, maternal psychopathology, and child behavior into consideration. DEDICATION This dissertation is lovingly dedicated to Jean and lrwin Becker, whose loving support throughout this process was given unconditionally and with a belief that, when finished, despite the obstacles that occurred, I would never regret any of the effort I put into my education. iv ACKNOWLEDGMENTS I would like to acknowledge the help of my committee Dr. H. E. Fitzgerald, Dr. Ellen Strommen, Dr. Ellen Whipple, and my chair, Dr. Michael C. Lambert for guiding me through the dissertation process. I would also like to thank Dr. Strommen and Dr. Whipple for discussing the diverse roles of children and their families from cross-cultural and within-in culture perspectives. To my chair, Dr. Michael C. Lambert, I would especially like to acknowledge how his excitement, interest, and commitment to children and their families from a global perspective have influenced my own work and interests. I would, in addition, like to thank Dr. Lauren J. Harris, who, while not officially on my committee, offered counsel, guidance, and support during the writing process. TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES LIST OF FIGURES INTRODUCTION AND LITERATURE REVIEW Theoretical Perspectives Socialization and Developmental Tasks: Early Childhood Socialization and Developmental Tasks: A Jamaican Perspective Influences on Socialization Practices Family Functioning Parent-Child Interaction GOALS Questions of Interest METHOD Participants Demographic Information Family Measures Maternal Measures Child Measures RESULTS Results for Models 1 and 2: Dysthymia Models Results for Models 3 and 4: Major Depression with Psychotic Feature Models DISCUSSION SES, Maternal Psychopathology and Family Cohesion Implications and Future Directions APPENDICES Appendix A: Frequency Tables for Variables in Analyzed and Archival Models Appendix B: Frequency Distributions for Variables in Analyzed and Archival Models Appendix C: Archival Models REFERENCES vi vii ix 17 20 25 29 38 38 41 41 42 49 57 63 69 69 74 78 79 96 111 114 Table Table Table Table Table Table Table Table Table Table Table Table Table Table Table Table Table 935999.“? 10: 11: 12: 13: 14: 15: 16: 17: LIST OF TABLES Descriptive Statistics Faces-II Dimensions BSI Dimensions for Depression Conners Factors Blvariate Correlations for all Variables in Models Frequency Table of Puzzle-Solving Scores from the McCarthy Scales of Child Development Frequency Table of Visual Factor Scores from the McCarthy Scales of Child Development Frequency Table of Conceptual Grouping Scores From the McCarthy Scales of Child Development Frequency Table of Verbal Factor Scores from the McCarthy Scales of Child Development Frequency Table of Mothers’ Educational Attainment Frequency Table of Parents’ Relationship Frequency Distribution of Mothers’ Age Frequency Table of Child IQ Scores Frequency Table of Study Children In Slx Basic Schools Frequency Table of Age of Study Children Frequency Table of Total Socioeconomic Status of Families of Study Children Frequency Table of lmmaturity Scores from the Conners Behavioral Scales vii 42 47 52 80 81 82 83 87 91 Table Table Table Table 18: 19: 20: 21: LIST OF TABLES (continued) Frequency Table of Hyperactivity Scale from the Conners Behavioral Scale Frequency Table of Family Cohesion Factor on the Faces Frequency Table of the Dysthymia Scores on the Brief Symptom Inventory Frequency Table of Psychotic Scores on the Brief Symptom Inventory viii 92 93 Figure Figure Figure Figure Figure Figure Figure Figure Figure Figure Figure Figure Figure Figure Flgure 399939 10: 11: 12: 13: 14: 15: LIST OF FIGURES Conceptual Relations Among Variables Model 1: Influence of SES, Maternal Dysthymia, and Family Cohesion on Child Outcome Model 2: Influence of SES, Maternal Dysthymia, and Family Cohesion on Child Outcome Model 3: Influence of SES, Major Depression with Psychotic Features, and Family Cohesion on Child Outcome Model 4: Influence of SES, Major Depression with Psychotic Features, and Family Cohesion on Child Outcome Frequency Distribution of Mothers’ Age Frequency Distribution of Parents’ Relationship Frequency Distribution of Mothers’ Education Frequency Distribution of Verbal Factor Scores from the McCarthy Scales of Child Development Frequency Distribution of Visual Factor Scores from the McCarthy Scales of Child Development Frequency Disb'ibution of Conceptual Grouping Scores from the McCarthy Scales of Child Development Frequency Distribution of Study Children at Basic Schools Frequency Distribution of Age of Study Children Frequency Distribution of Total Socioeconomic Status of Families of Study Children Frequency Distribution of Immaturity Scores from the Connors Behavioral Scale ix 65 97 98 100 101 102 103 104 105 108 Figure 16: Figure 17: Figure 18: Figure 19: Figure 20: Figure 21: LIST OF FIGURES (CONTINUED) Frequency Distribution of Hyperactivity Factor Scores from the Connors Behavioral Scale Frequency Distribution of Family Cohesion Factor Scores on the FACES Frequency Distribution of Dysthymia Factor Scores on the Brief Symptom Inventory Frequency Distribution of Psychotic Factor Scores on the Brief Symptom Inventory Archival Model: Influence of SES, Maternal Psychotic Behavior, and Family Cohesion on Child Outcome Archival Model: Influence of SES, Matamal Dysthymia, and Family Cohesion on Child Outcome 107 108 109 110 112 113 INTRODUCTION AND LITERATURE REVIEW Psychologists, educators, clinicians, and policy makers recognize the importance of early childhood (i.e., ages 3—8) as a time for encouraging, building, and strengthening the competencies needed for the child’s successful transition from home to school (Pianta, 1999; Stipek, 2001; White, 1995). The ability to regulate one’s own behavior, either alone or with the support of others, and the ability to use language to communicate with others are two of the most important cont-ibutors to a successful school experience for young children. Competencies in the developmental areas discussed above help scaffold the child’s successful integration into the social world (Richards, 1974; Vygotsky, 1930/1978). Failure, however, to master these skills early in life may be associated with serious behavioral, emotional, and Ieaming difficulties at all stages of life - early childhood, middle and later childhood, adolescence, and adulthood (Committee on Early Childhood Pedagogy, Commission on Behavioral and Social Sciences and Education, 8 National Research Council, 2001). Three of the factors already generally known to play important roles in the process were chosen for study: socioeconomic status (Keating and Hertzman 1999; Stipek, 2001), maternal psychopathology (Campbell, 1987; Campbell 8 Ewing, 1990), and family functioning; Campbell 8 Pierce, 1996; Campbell, Pierce, March 8 Ewing, 1991, 1994). Although most research studies on behavior regulation and language ability have been conducted in industrialized nations (Durbrow, 1999) interest in early childhood education extends as well to developing countries (Morrison 8 Milner, 1995). Indeed, the Committee on Early Childhood Pedagogy and collaborators (2001) has posited that universal early childhood education will be a reality in the not-too-distant future, thus making research in early childhood and its implications for education a topic for universal study. Jamaica is one example of a developing country that has seen very few empirical studies on children (Lambert, Weisz, 8 Knight, 1989a). As a result, educators, physicians, psychologists, and others who work with young children rely on research conducted in developed and more economically stable countries (Lambert et al., 1989a). There are limits, however, to the extent to which we can generalize from one country to another, especially from developed to developing countries (Geertz, 1973). The vast differences in economic circumstances exert a powerful effect, both directly and indirectly, on the material, social, educational, and medical resources that are available to the child, and thus have immediate as well as Iong-tenn effects on the child’s development (Keating and Hertzman 1999). For another example, there are cultural differences in many practices and beliefs that affect the socialization process, including the development and expression of emotion and behavior regulation (Lambert et al., 1989a; Valsiner 1989). Such differences also extend to rules guiding when and how to express strong feelings (Lambert, Knight, Taylor, 8 Achenbach, 1994; Ekman 8 Davidson, 1994), rules governing parent-child interaction (Bronfenbrenner 1986; Durbrow 1999), beliefs about what is most important in the socialization process (Bronfenbrenner 1986; Durbrow 1999), and how to teach children new skills (Rogoff, Mistry, Goncu, 8 Mosier, 1993). In Jamaica, there Is a serious need for better information. In 1996, for example, the Jamaican Teachers Association asked the Chief Education Officer of the Ministry of Education to help them understand and manage the severe behavior problems that they were experiencing with children in all Jamaican schools (Morrison, Ipsa, 8 Milner, 1998). On previous occasions the Basic School [non-governmental schools for preschoolers (ages 3-6)] teachers voiced a concern about their children’s lack of curiosity and their reluctance to participate in new activities (Morrison et al., 1998). These are especially worrisome inasmuch as curiosity and readiness to participate are normally characteristic of young children and indicate an interest in Ieaming (Committee on Early Childhood Pedagogy et al., 2001). The current study, therefore, has a two-fold goal. The first goal is to assess the contribution of socioeconomic status, maternal psychopathology, and family functioning to behavior regulation and language ability in young Jamaican children. The second goal is to determine whether the relation among these potential contributors to child outcome is the same as that reported in the United States, Canada, and Europe. The second goal is important for two reasons. First, even though the current study will not measure the effect of culmre directly, the result can provide insight about the patterns of socioeconomic status, maternal psychopathology, and family functioning on child outcome. The current study, therefore, can contribute to the scientific dialogue on universal versus context-specific aspect of behavior in relation to these particular maternal, family, and socioeconomic variables. Second, by enhancing our understanding of how these influences work in Jamaica, we can begin to build a foundation of knowledge for Jamaican clinicians, educators, and policy makers to draw on as they grapple with the complex social issues in their country and as they plan programs for early childhood education. The literature review to follow begins with a presentation of theoretical perspectives for the current study. It then reviews theory and research on the influence of culture in psychological research. After that, it examines developmental tasks of early childhood, followed by a discussion of the socialization process and those factors that influence it outcome - socioeconomic status, maternal psychopathology, and family functioning. Special attention is given to a discussion of Jamaican culture and it influence on the expectations, beliefs, values, and practices within the family. Figure 1 present a model of the relations to be considered. Child Behavior Regulation Maternal Psychopathology Socioeconomic Status (SES) F Child Verbal Ability I Family Functioning Child Puzzle-Solving Skill Figure 1: Conceptual Relations Among Variables Theoretical Persge_<_:tives Socioeconomic Psychosocial Int_egration The overall perspective that guides this research is called Socioeconomic Psychosocial Intgration, or SEP. SEP is not itelf a theory of human development. Rather, it is a call for a new way to study and interpret influences on a call for a new way to study and interpret influences on developmental outcome. It posit that a successful theory must integrate knowledge across many disciplines. For psychology, some of the most important disciplines to integrate include education, sociology, social work, epidemiology, cultural anthropology, neurobiology, and neuroscience. SEP also sees conventional variables such as socioeconomic status to stand for much more than, for example, material resources, and instead to be a marker for highly complex processes that begin before birth and continue across the life-course. These include such identifiable social circumstances as attitudes and ballet about education and the safety of one’s neighborhood and that integrate neurobiological factors with experience [see Keating 8 Hertzman (1999) for a full discussion]. SEP also incorporates the concept of the “social gradient” to indicate the complex relation between socioeconomic status as it is conventionally understood and the relation between socioeconomic status and it multiple influences on psychosocial functioning [e.g., Case, Griffin, 8 Kelly (1999, 2001) and Tremblay (1999) for a full discussion]. Countries or communities may have sharp social gradient indicating extremes of wealth and poverty, such as are found in Jamaica, or smoother gradient indicating less extreme differences. These different gradient can have very different effect on such psychosocial outcomes as maternal psychopathology, family cohesion (e.g., the ability of the family to function harmoniously), and child behavior problems. As statistical indicators, the gradient operate on the population level and not on the individual level so that psychosocial outcomes like those listed above will not necessarily be true for individuals, even though they may describe the population (Keating 8 Hartman, 1999). Social gradient, flierefore, affect families at the populafion level and families are embedded in cultural beliefs and practices that may or may not be influenced by socioeconomic factors (Keating and Hartman 1999), the next section will discuss theoretical perspectives concerning families and culture. Culture, Context, and Families Acknowledging the influence of culture and traditions on family, children, and schooling is important in understanding the relationships and expectancies among parent, family, and children (Durbrow, 1999; Geert, 1973; Lambert, Knight, Taylor, 8 Newell, 1993; Rogoff et al., 1993). One theoretical stance taken toward the inclusion of cultural similarities and differences and their influence on child or family outcomes is cultural- contextual theory (Cole, 1999), a theory that acknowledges the influence and interplay among culture, context, and developmental outcome. In addition, scholars of the family (Bronfenbrenner 1986) generally acknowledge that families are organized to perform specific tasks and functions as prescribed by cultural and societal norms. One such task is to help integrate the child into the social world (Richards, 1974). Scholars and researchers also acknowledge the waiter of influences on the quality of family functioning and, in particular, childrearing. One of the approaches acknowledging these complexities is family systems theory. There are many such theories. All of them acknowledge the varieties of ways that families may organize, accommodate to it members, and influence child outcome. According to the structural family systems theory (Minuchin 1992), for the family to fulfill it many obligations, it must form subsystems, each one with it specific role within the overall family organization. The parental subsystem generally assumes the major responsibility for child rearing, including teaching and shaping social behavior. Much of the extant research on child outcome focuses on parent-child interaction, often from the perspective of the nuclear family, found most often in the United States, Canada, and Europe. Families in Jamaica, however, often do not have the same organizational pattern as families in industrialized countries (Durbrow, 1999; GopauI-McNicol, 1998, 1999), and therefore, may influence children differently. Culture and the Research Process As previously stated, the importance of cultural influences on development is now acknowledged in much psychological research. Often, however, the intellectual stance toward the characteristics of culture and their influence on the developmental process is not addressed directly (Durbrow, 1999; GopauI-McNicoI, 1999). Acknowledged in word, but not in content or approach to the research question, the use of culture as a variable can easily become a cliche, while the research process, including questions asked and the interpretation of result continues unchanged or unquestioned (Rogler, 1999). There are at least two ways that consideration of culture affect die research process. The first addresses the culture-general and culture- specific nature of the research question (Lambert et al., 1989a)(e.g., Do parent in all cultures have the same goals for the socialization of their children?). There is convincing evidence that socialization goals are strongly influenced by cultural values and beliefs. For example, Gonzalez- Ramos, Zayas, and Cohen (1998) found that American mothers placed high value on creativity and independence in the socialization of their preschool children, whereas for Puerto Rican mothers, what was important was loyalty to family and respect for ofl‘lers. From a culture-general perspective, both Puerto Rican and American mothers accepted responsibility for the socialization of their young children, but from a culture-specific perspective, their goals were different and, most likely, would result in different types of guidance and interaction with their children. Views about the similarity or differences among cultures also influence the research process itelf. One model of cultural influences on the research process posit three ways to conceptualize the influence of culture on development (Bukowski 8 Sippola, 1998; Geert, 1973). The first way is the details model, which states that cultures differ only in small details, so that instrument standardized for one culture may be used without modification across cultures and not diminish outcome validity. Much early work in psychology implicitly accepted this stance about cultural influences (Durbrow 1999) and, therefore, made broad generalizations of psychological findings across cultures. This approach can be seen as a 100% culture-general stance. A second approach, on the other end of the continuum, takes an essentialist perspective. This approach assumes that there are no similarities across cultures, so that assessment instrument cannot be modified in any way but must be created specifically for the population under study. This perspective, if taken to an extreme, does not permit cross-cultural research because nothing from one culture can be generalized to another. It is an extreme case of culture-specificity. A third model is the local knowledge model, which acknowledges that many commonalities across cultures but posit that local knowledge and belief strongly influence the developmental process (e.g., parent-child 10 interaction) and, therefore, the developmental outcome. Advocates for this model argue that for validity, information about the population being studied should be incorporated in the assessment instrument. Prior work by Lambert and his colleagues (Lambert at al., 1989a; Lambert et al., 1994; Lambert, Knight, Taylor, 8 Achenbach, 1996; Lambert 8 Lyubansky, 1999) draws from existing assessment instrument [e.g., Achenbach’s Child Behavior Check List (CBCL)] but modifies the questions based on contributions from clinic referred Jamaican youth, their parent, teachers, other adult who referred them for treatment, and from the contributions of clinicians who treat Jamaican children (Lambert et al., 1989a; Lambert at al., 1994; Lambert at al., 1996; Lambert 8 Lyubansky, 1999) The CBCL was then modified to reflect the Jamaican thresholds for problem behaviors as well as behaviors thought to be problematic. For example, for a Jamaican adult, a child who throws stones for any reason has a behavior problem; for an American adult, the same behaviors displayed by a child would not ordinarily be a cause for concern. Although United States and Jamaican parent, therefore, would agree that children have behavior problems, they differ in what constitutes appropriate and inappropriate behavior. The researcher who fails to take these differences into account and generalizes across cultures runs the risk of overlooking or misclassifying psychological difficulties in one culture not so identified In another. 11 The current study assesses Jamaican children, mothers, and families, and thus is a within-culture study. From a theoretical perspective, however, both culture-general and culture-specific perspectives are taken into account, although no direct cross-cultural comparisons can be made. The literature review to follow examines research predominantly from the United States, Canada, and Europe, and the question of whether established culture-general result will apply equally well to Jamaica in relation to the effect of maternal psychopaflrology, family functioning, and child outcome remain to be seen. Socialization and Developmental Tasks: Early Childhood §oclalization Socialization has two goals: to teach the child how to function in the home and in the larger community, and second, to teach the child to balance it needs with those of others, whether they are parent, extended family, classmates, or friends. Several skills are needed to achieve these goals, including behavior regulation, emotion regulation, and the use of language to communicate ideas, thought, and feelings to others, and mastering them is a complex process (Richards 1974; Rogoff 1990; White 1995). Many factors influence this process (Bronfenbrenner 1986), including, but not limited to, cultural values and beliefs that effect adult expectations of the child’s emotion and behavior regulation (Lambert et al., 1989a) and the quality of the parent-child interaction (Bronfenbrenner 1986; Gotbnan, Kat, 8 Hooven, 1997; Maccoby 1992). 12 In early childhood, sociallzafion of behavior regulation, emotion regulation, and language is initially acquired in the family. In the family, fire child also Ieams to use adult as resources for asking questions and problem solving and to use language to express feelings and to communicate ideas (Nelson 1996; Rogoff, Baker-Sennatt, Lacasa, 8 Goldsmifir, 1995; White 1995). In addition, fire child learns fire appropriate ' ways to inhibit or express behaviors and feelings and how to use firis knowledge wlfir family and peers (Maccoby, 1992). Competencies in firese areas help to scaffold fire child’s transition from home to school (Plant, 1999) and are associated wifir positive peer and adult relafionships and with academic success, beginning in early childhood and continuing firrough adolescence (Plant 1999; Sfipek 2001). Failure to gain competence may be associated wifir serious behavioral, emotional, and Ieaming difficulties at later stages in life, from middle and later childhood, to adolescence, to adulfirood (Campbell, 1990; Campbell, 1995; Campbell 8 Pierce, 1996; Caspi, Moffitt, Newman, 8 Silva, 1997). Alfirough fire skills needed for successful socialization are inifially learned and practiced in fire family, and alfirough socialization begins in early infancy and early childhood (Keenan and Shaw, 1994; Maccoby, 1992; Richards, 1974) most studies of parental and environmental contributors to child outcome have started in middle childhood [e.g., see Zucker 8 Gomberg (1986) for a review of fire literature on children of alcoholics]. More recent research, however, has begun wifir preschool-age children 13 (e.g., Campbell, 1995 for a review; Radke-Yarrow, 1998; Zucker 8 Fitgerald, 1991) and is providing substantial evidence that early behavior patterns are related to later developmental outcome. Campbell and Ewing (1990), for example, found firat children who at age firree had difficulfies with behavior and emofion regulation (e.g., aggressive and impulsive behavior) were significanfiy more likely firan children wifirout firese difficulties to have behavior problems at ages 6 and 9. Looking even farfirer across the life-course, Caspi and his colleagues (Caspi et al., 1997) found first behavior problems in 3-year-old children were associated wifir later adult psychopafirology. These studies provide evidence firat behavior problems in early childhood may influence later child and adult outcome, firus highlighting fire importance of recognizing and understanding specific influences on fire socialization process. Because most Ieaming occurs In a social context, fire Ieaming process involves co-particlpation of fire parent and teachers as fire child learns emotion regulation, behavior regulation, and language development [for a discussion of co-participation see Lave 8 Wenger (1991 )1. Emotion Regulation. The ability to regulate emotions has antecedent in infancy and toddlerhood (e.g., Field, 1995) but becomes more critical in early childhood as fire child’s interacfions with ofirers, especially peers, increase. As previously stated, firere are cultural and family expectations for fire appropriate times and ways to express emotions such as anger and sadness (Ekman 8 Davidson, 1994; Lambert et 14 al., 1996; Lambert, Lyubansky, 8 Achenbach, 1998; Lambert et al., 1994; Lambert, Puig, Lyubansky, 8 Rowan, 2001a;). The socializafion of emotion regulation requires at least firree skills. The first skill requires fire recognition and understanding of one’s own internal feelings. The second skill is to learn socially appropriate ways to respond to fire experience of emotion (e.g., Gottrnan et al., 1997). The firird skill, which complement fire first, is to accurately interpret fire emotion responses of anofirer. This is a key component in beginning and maintaining successful peer relationships. Children who have difficulty interpreting ofirers’ emotion responses often misinterpret firose responses as negative or hosfile, and, firerefore, are more likely to respond wifir anger or distress. Studies show firat fire ability to correctly read fire emotion responses of others is a key component in initiafing and maintaining successful peer relationships (Crick 8 Dodge, 1994; Crick 8 Dodge, 1996). Behavior Rggulafion. Behavior regulafion is closely related to emotion regulation because behavior regulation often depends on fire ability to manage emotion responses in a variety of situations (e.g., at home, at school, playing wifir peers). Children who can regulate fireir behavioral responses when working andlor playing wifir ofirers maintain friendships and have more successful experiences within fire peer group than children who have difficulty wifir firese same skills (Crick 8 Dodge, 1994; Crick 8 Dodge, 1996). Parent, as co-participant in fire process, help fireir child gain competence in behavior regulafion. In addifion to 15 direct instruction (Rogoff et al., 1993), research shows firat parental warrnfir, sensitivity, responsiveness, aufiroritative child-rearing styles, and child-centered management techniques all help fire child to develop behavior and emotion regulation (Gralinski 8 Kopp, 1993). In one such study, Gralinskl and Kopp (1993) examined middle-class, well-educated parent and fireir children, and a developmental fimetable of parental expectations emerged. For toddlers, parental expectafions centered on safety and protection (e.g., “don’t put a fork in fire electric socket”); for preschoolers, parent formalized expectations for proper behavior at mealtime, self-care (e.g., brushing teefir), and for respecting fire person and property of ofirers. As fire children grew older, fire mofirers furfirer elaborated rules and expectafions coincided wifir fire child’s entrance into school, where expectations of behavior regulafion would also come from teachers. Language Development. The use of language marks a key transifion from toddlerhood to early childhood. Whereas toddlers often communicate anger or displeasure wifir ofirers by physically aggressive means, in early childhood Ieaming to use language to communicate feelings is an important developmental task (e.g.,Gottman et al., 1997). The child is also Ieaming how to use language in formal (e.g., school) and Informal (e.g., home) situations, which helps fire child modulate emofions and behavior. Difficulty with language and communication in young children is often associated wifir problems in behavior and emotion regulation (Beltchman, 16 Wilson, Brownlie, 8 Waters, 1996; Benasich 8 Curtiss, 1993; Carson, Klee, Perry, Muskina, 8 Donagy, 1998). In sum, fire core of self-control, defined as fire ability to act according to fire expectations of fire caregiver, even when fire caregiver is not present (Gralinski 8 Kopp, 1993), is fire ability to inhibit Inifial impulses so firat a proper form of acfion can be initiated (Maccoby, 1992). Thus as children learn to regulate emotion and behavior and to use language effectively, their ability to manage aggression, empafirize wifir ofirers, and act appropriately in a variety of settings increases. Socializafion and Developmental Tasks: A Jamaican Persgctive As previously noted, much of a child’s early socialization occurs in fire family, most often between parent and child. Cultural values and beliefs, firerefore, about fire appropriate ways to socialize children affect how parent approach firis process (Durbrow 1999; Rogoff et al., 1995). Among cultural variations identified in meefing fire responsibilities for child rearing are who takes fire primary role for day-to-day child-care, what is appropriate discipline, and who disciplines fire child, family attitudes toward schooling, including what is valued and what behavior is considered appropriate. Also, fire type of family structure adopted in a culfirre or subculture influences cultural variations in child rearing (Wint 8 Brown, 1987). In Jamaica, for example, fire mofirer takes fire main responsibility for child rearing. A grandmofirer or aunt, however, will most likely become fire 17 primary caregiver if fire mofirer is unable to do so. Maternal figures are also direcfiy involved wifir children regardless of maternal availability. Socialization values for Jamaican children center around obedience, sharing, and respect for ofirers (Durbrow, 1999; Gopaul-McNicol, 1999; Leo- Rhynie, 1993; Morrison 8 Milner, 1995; Rogoff et al., 1995; Valsiner, 1989). Beginning in early childhood, Jamaican parent, firerefore, expect fireir children to obey and follow fireir directives (Gopaul-McNicol, 1999; Morrison 8 Milner, 1995). When children are disobedient, fire two most common disciplinary mefirods are spanking and wifirdrawal of love (Morrison 8 Milner, 1995). Alfirough spanking and love wifirdrawal are commonly used, Jamaican mofirers are also emotionally warm, attentive, and nurturing toward fireir children (Grant 1974, 1984). Jamaican mofirers believe firat it is fire combinafion firat prevent fire child from becoming “spoiled” and that prepares fire child for fire behavior expected in school. The first school exposure most Jamaicans experience is fire “Basic School.” This type of education is firerefore described next. Early Childhood Education in Jamaica Basic Schools Jamaica has several forms of early childhood education (Johnson 8 Brown“, 1995), many of which come under fire supervision of fire education officers of fire Jamaican Ministry of Early Childhood Unit. One such program is basic school, a program for children ages 3- to 6- years old firat is financed from small contribufions from parent and, more recenfiy, from 18 government subsidies for accredited schools (Johnson 8 Brown, 1995). These schools serve 82% of fire total population of 4- to 6- year olds, the majority of who are in fire low-income strata of society (Johnson 8 Brown, 1995). In 1985, fire Ministry of Education indorsed an eclectic child- centered curriculum firat draws from Montessori, Froebel, Piaget, Bruner, and Pestalozzi, fire goal of which is to teach fire child to work independenfiy and creatively (Johnson 8 Brown, 1995; Morrison et al., 1998). Many Basic Schools are understaffed wifir child-adult ratios that greafiy restrict fire amount of attention given to any one child. Also, many classrooms do not have enough space to accommodate fire number of children who attend (James, 1977; Stebbins, 1973). This also restrict fire amount of individual and small group work firat can be done (Johnson 8 Brown, 1995). Teachers and Children in Basic School. Basic School tachers are para-professionals wifir low levels of academic preparation and wifir litfie or no pro-teaching training. Most of fireir knowledge about children and curriculum firerefore comes from in-service training. This lack of academic preparation, bofir in curriculum and child development, may, in part, explain fire result of two evaluations, one in 1986, fire ofirer in 1995, firat found firat Basic School teachers had difficulty implementing fire curriculum endorsed by fire Ministry of Education (Johnson 8 Brown, 1995). Anofirer explanation may be related to fire behavioral expectations l9 for children. Jamaican children are expected to be obedient to aufirority bofir at home and at school. This is a value first bofir parent and teachers hold as important (Morrison et al., 1998). In addifion, Jamaican parent place great importance on success in school and firerefore expect fire child to be obedient, respectful, and cooperative, not only wifir firemselves, as already noted, but also wifir fire teacher and ofirer children. In school, for example, children are expected to have fire self-regulation necessary to wait fireir turn during fire school day (Lambert at al., 1996). Jamaican parent also endorse academic learning (i.e., reading, writing, and arlfirmetic) over ofirer more creafive aspect of fire curriculum (e.g., painting, playing wifir blocks). These subject are taught by rote Ieaming, rafirer firan in an experienfial, constructivist manner, mefirods endorsed by fire Ministry of Education. The curriculum, however, encourages creativity and play in fire classroom and, firerefore, conflict wifir expectations for obedience, self-regulation, and academic Ieaming may be difficult for fire child and fire teacher to resolve. Influences on Socialization Pr_acfices One influence on fire context and content of setfings is socioeconomic status (Brooks-Gunn, Klebanov, Llaw, 8 Duncan, 1995; Stipek, 2001; Stipek 8 Ryan, 1997), fire next topic to be discussed. Socioeconomic Status. As previously discussed, socioeconomic status (SES) is often used to identify an individual’s or family’s place in fire social and economic hierarchy of a community or country (Brooks-Gunn, 20 Duncan, Britto, 1999; Duncan, Brooks-Gunn, 8 Klebanov, 1994; Duncan 8 Young, W. J., 1998). In fire United States, social class is usually divided into firree divisions (upper, middle, and lower), wifir classification based on a combinafion of education, occupation, and income. In firese respect, SES is also a marker for a variety of associated medical, literacy, and recreafion resources and opportunities, as well as associated practices, values, and beliefs (Leo-Rhynie 8 Hamilton 1993). Low SES also may be a significant indicator of difficulties for fire child and for fire family as a whole (T amowski 8 Rohrbeck, 1993). In most countries, for example, SES is negatively relatd to adult psychopafirology and positively related to psychological health for children and adult alike (T arnowski 8 Rohrbeck, 1993; Dohrenwend, 1990; Dohrenwend, Levav, Shrout, 8 Schwart, 1992). fiioeconomic Status in Jamaica. As previously discussed, in developing countries SES is more difficult to define then it is in fire developed countries of fire West (Leo-Rhynie and Hamilton, 1993), and, in fact, may not reflect fire same factors idenfified as salient in fire developed world. Alfirough Jamaica has many natural resources, it also has serious economic problems marked by 20% unemployment, a heavy foreign debt, and high inflation. Currenfiy, alfirough fire economy is growing slowly, inflation is increasing dramatically, firus contribufing to a declining standard of living for many. The measurement of SES is difficult in any developing country in large part because of fireso kinds of economic problems are common, not 21 only to Jamaica but also firroughout fire developing world (Lambert et al., 2001a). The measurement of SES in such countries, firerefore, should not rely on income or professional status as proxies (McLoyd 8 Ceballo, 1998). In Jamaica, furfirerrnore, fire deterrninafion of SES from reported individual or family income is still more problematic because many families receive substantial aid in fire form of gift from relatives outide fire country. This aid, however, is measurable because it normally is used for fire purchase of material possessions for fire home (e.g., refrigerator, television, radio). For firese reasons, in Jamaica, instead of using fire more convenfional economic indicos of SES, a better measure is “wealfir,” defined as fire total economic resources available to children and families. As so defined, “wealfir,” has been shown to be a better predictor of emotional well-being firan occupational status or income (McLoyd 8 Ceballo, 1998). Parental educafion, whefirer based on parent’ educafion (Amato, 2000) or only fire mofirer’s education (Doucette-Gates, Brooks- Gunn, 8 Chase-Lansdale, 1998), is also a predictor of well being in children. Recent studies have begun to compare fire Hollingshead SES index (Hollingshead, 1975) wifir measures of wealfir (e.g., money, material possessions) as well as availability of services and adequacy of basic and ofirer resources such as food and shelter (Lambert et al., 2001 a). These studies, for example, find firat firese altematlve measures are just as good 22 a predictor ofpsychopafirology as is fire Hollingshead index (1975) (e.g., Rose, 1998). Socioeconomic Status and Child Outcome For children, low SES is associated wifir a higher risk for extemallzing and internalizing behavior as well as a higher risk for peer rejection (Raver 8 Leadbeater, 1999). When comparing preschool children from a low and middle SES group, Ramsey (1988) used two procedures to assess peer relationships and problem-solving skill. One asked children to respond to social problem situations; fire ofirer used a sociometric assessment of friends in fire classroom. The result was firat children from fire low SES group responded more aggressively when seeking a solution to fire problem situations and were also rated by fireir teachers as less socially competent. In sum, fire low SES children had more conflict over object, had fewer problem-solving skills, and had more aggressive interaction styles. In examining longitudinal data from fire Infant Healfir and Development Program for low birth weight babies (N = 895), Duncan, et al., (1994) found firat poverty status and family income were strongly related to children’s cognitive development and behavior. Children who at age 5 lived in more affluent neighborhoods functioned at higher intellectual levels firan firose children who remained at or near fire poverty level. Stipek (2001) found that poverty in early childhood is associated wifir poorer performance in adulfirood in literacy and ofirer school related skills. 23 In fire longitudinal Mater University Study of Pregnancy, children whose families remained poor across all firree assessment periods (antenatal, 6 monfirs postnatal, and 5 years of age) had fire highest number of behavior problems. Murphy-Bennan, Levesque, and Berrnan (1996) also found firat fire caregiving of a mofirer who was struggling wifir financial hardship was often like firat of a clinically depressed mofirer. To furfirer understand fire relation between SES and maternal depression, fire next secfion will explore fire relation between SES and mental healfir. Socioeconomic Status, Mental HealfirI and Family Funcfioning. A United Nations sponsored epidemiological study found a strong relationship between SES and mental illness (i.e., fire lower fire level of SES, fire higher fire incidence of mental illness). This study also identified some risk factors accompanying low SES to be chronic and acute stress, lack of adequate social support, and restricted sense of control of one’s life (Murphy-Barman et al., 1996). This finding lends support to Sameroff (1975) who posit firat it is not just SES firat contributes to problematic outcome but it is also fire number of other risk factors firat accompany low SES firat contribute to fire development of mental illness, firus contributing to problems in fire individual and fire family. There is also a body of research firat suggest firat race, especially being of minority status, contributes to poor mental healfir outcome. To explore firis relationship, drawing on epidemiological data from 1,648 American White and 450 Black adult, Biafora (1995) examined fire relation 24 between SES, depression, and race. In fire initial analysis, Blacks had higher depression scores firan did Whites; however, when SES (occupafional status, education level, and income) was partialed out, race was not an independent predictor of depression. SES, not race, firus carried fire predictive power for depression. It is important, firerefore, to acknowledge fire complexity of examining fire effect of SES on child or adult outcome and family functioning. Alfirough fire evidence often shows low SES, as defined convenfionally, by education, income, and occupafion, to be a predictor of child behavior problems and adult psychopafirology, recent studies suggest it is not low SES itelf but fire number of associated risk factors firat actually contribute to fire problems (McLoyd 8 Ceballo, 1998; Sameroff, 1975; Sameroff 8 Seifer, 1990, 1995; Seifer 8 Sameroff, 1987). And, as previously discussed, Keafing and Hartman (1999) emphasize fire influence of fire socioeconomic psychological gradient on individual, family, and community outcome. In sum, it is important to acknowledge firat SES is often a marker for ofirer difficulties firat may confront fire individual or family. Family Functioning Families are embedded in a larger social system, evolve over time, and vary wifirin and among cultural groups. As previously mentioned, fire family provides fire early socialization experiences for fire child to learn family norms, values, and behavioral expectations. Organization of firese 25 experiences into schemas and script helps fire child to anticipate and respond to day-to-day expectations of fire family. Because family members relate to one anofirer on a close emotional level such firat a change in one person affect fire behavior of anofirer, family cohesion, conflict, and adaptability are related, positively or negatively, to child and adult well-being and mental healfir. Family cohesion ls defined as fire “emotional bonding firat family members have toward one anofirer and fire degree of individual autonomy firey experience” (Olson, Spenke, and Russell, 1979). Low levels of cohesion thus may have deleterious effect on family members. For example, GormanSmifir, Tolan, Zelli, 8 Huesmann (1996) found firat families of violent delinquent exhibited less family cohesion and less involvement firan parent of non-violent adolescent. I Family adaptability as defined by Olson et al. (1979) is “fire ability of fire marital or family system to change it power structure, role relationships, and relationship rules in response to situational and developmental stress.” The hypofiresis is firat fire more adaptable a family is to change, whefirer positive (e.g., birfir of a healfiry child) or negative (e.g., loss of substantial family income), fire higher it level of functioning. Thus, higher levels of cohesion and adaptability are related to higher levels of positive family functioning. 26 Family Cohesion and Child Outcome In a study of American preschool children and fireir parent, Bullock and Pennington (1988) found firat parent’ self-perceptions of family cohesion related positively to fireir child’s sense of fireir own competence, to fire quality of fire child’s friendships, and to fire teacher’s perception of fire child’s competence. Lindahl (1998) found firat family cohesion was one factor firat distinguished progressively among firree groups of boys (ages 7-1 1): one group wifir attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), anofirer wifir opposifional defiant disorder (ODD), and still anofirer wifir ADHD and ODD. For bofir ODD groups, lack of family cohesiveness played an important role in family functioning. The families of boys wifir no behavior problems (control group) and fire families of boys wifir ADHD had average to above-average family cohesion. ~ Cola and Jordan (1989), however, have criticized assessment of family cohesion and adaptability as too general, in ofirer words, firat some part of fire family (subsystems) may be more cohesive or adaptable firan ofirers. Taking a different perspective, however, Johnson, Cowan, and Cowan (1999) have challenged fire view firat evaluating subsystems (e.g., mofirer—child, fafirer-child, mofirer-fafirer) account for fire variance in fire reporting of child behavior problems (e.g. externalizing and internalizing behavior), and firat to fully understand child behavior problems, understanding how fire family works togefirer is necessary. 27 Families in Jamaica The population of Jamaica primarily consist of descendant of “British-owned” slaves from fire Ashanti, Yoruba, Ibo, and Fanfi tribes of Africa (Brice-Baker, 1996). While efirnic groups from ofiror world regions such as Europe, Asia, and Middle-Eastem nations are represented in fire populafion, fireir gene pools are often mixed wifir one anofirer and firose of i African-Jamaicans. This is reflected in fire Jamaican national motto “Out of Many People.” Thus, fire cultural customs of Jamaica, including family customs primarily reflect African-British efiros (see Lambert 8 Lyubansky, 1999). For example, fire patriarchical family structure where much of fire power rest in fire fafirer is considered ideal family structure in Jamaica. Neverfireless, fire matrifocal structure and it emphasis on fire widely extended family wifirin fire context of fire community firat is evident in fire tribes from which Jamaican families originate predominates Jamaican family structure (Rutter, Yule, Morton, 8 Bagley, 1975). The African legacy and it focus on survival of fire group are similar to firat found in African-Americans. However, it stands in contrast wifir European-based U.S. ideals where autonomy and interpersonal competition prevails. Like many African-American families who share fire same heritage as Jamaicans (Lambert et al., 1999), firis legacy promotes cohesiveness wifir Jamaican communities and especially in families (see Hohn, 1996). Thus, individuals wifirin most Jamaican family structures strive to maintain cohesiveness and ward off threat to firis ideal. 28 Extremely high family cohesiveness for which most Jamaicans strive may be considered as enmeshed by external observers (GopauI-McNicol, 1998), but to most Jamaicans firis quality is viewed as critical to adequate and ideal family functioning. Support for firis nofion is documented in research on Jamaican immigrant where family support, an important aspect of cohesion, was found to buffer fire stress associated wifir migrafion to, and stress associated wifir life in fire United States (Adams, 1989). This finding was. replicated In a recent study, which indicated a positive relafionship between Family Cohesion and adequate psychological functioning in immigrant Jamaican children (Hohn, 1996). Furfirerrnore, a recent study on family functioning and child psychological adjustment in Jamaica indicated a posifive relafionship between cohesion and intellectual development and a negafive relafionship between firis predictor and child psychopafirology (Lambert, Schmitt, Samms-Vaughn, Russ, Lewis, Lancaster, 8 Orellana et al., 2001b). Parent-Child lnteracfion There are many influences on fire parent-child interaction wifirin fire context of fire family. One of fire most important is fire belief system guiding fire socialization process (Hastings 8 Rubin, 1999). As previously noted, Jamaican parent strongly believe firat children should be obedient and not question fire authority of parent or teachers. Also, as previously discussed, Jamaican mofirers believe firat firey can best guide fireir 29 children’s behavior wifir a combination of physical punishment and high levels of nurturance. Physical punishment. an emphasis on obedience, and an emphasis on not questioning aufirority are often associated wifir an aufiroritarian style of parent-child interaction (Baumrind, 1971). For all firese reasons, such children often have difficulty in school. They also show externalizing behavior, which, along wlfir internalizing behavior, is one of fire two most common child problems discussed wifirin fire context of parent-child interactions. Extemalizing behavior is also fire most frequenfiy studied child behavior problem (Rofirbaum 8 Weisz, 1994) and it presence is often associated wifir fire following caregiving qualifies: absence of approval, absence of guidance, absence of motivational strategies, absence of synchrony, and presence of coercive control. These caregiving qualifies are also found in mofirers who are depressed (Campbell, 1990) anofirer known risk factor for troubled child outcome. Maternal Psychogfirolggy Many research studies have linked maternal psychopafirology to child behavior problems (Dodge, 1990), and fire most commonly studied form of maternal psychopafirology is depression (Rutter, 1995), a mood disorder. Depression is now acknowledged to be heterogeneous and not homogeneous as long supposed. In Western literature, fire common use of fire term depression refers to a state of sadness, dejection, or lowering of spirit. Sadness, however, does not necessarily correspond to a clinical diagnosis of depression. 30 Therefore, before reviewing fire literature on maternal depression and child outcome, definitions and descriptions of fire types of depression commonly studied are discussed. %phoria. Dysphoria is commonly used to refer to sadness and dojecfion and is not, according to DSM-IV (1994) criteria, clinically diagnosable as depression but may, neverfireless, interfere wifir normal day-to-day functioning and social interaction (Field, 1995). Qypfirymia and Major Depressive Disorder. Dysfirymia and Major Depressive Disorder are differonfiated based on severity, chronicity, and persistence (American Psychiatric Association, 1994). Dysfirymia, for example, requires fire presence of a depressed mood for more days firan not over a period of two years. Major depressive disorder, however, requires firat fire depressed mood must be present almost every day for two weeks. Differentiating between firese two types of depression is complicated (American Psychiatric Association, 1994) because bofir disorders share many symptoms. Dysfirymia is seen as a chronic, less severe depression firat last over a period of years, whereas major depression is an acute depression firat can be distinguished from a person’s usual behavior. For a more detailed descripfion of symptoms and differential diagnostic criteria for mood disorders see DSM-IV (American Psychiatric Association, 1994) pages 341 -392. Major Depressive Disorder wifir Psychotic Symptoms. As discussed above, major depressive disorder requires firat fire depressed mood must 3] be present almost every day for two weeks. In Major Depressive Disorder wifir Psychofic Symptoms, fire psychotic symptoms may relate to fire mood disorder. Examples of symptoms, drawn from fire DSM-IV (American Psychiatric Association, 1994, p. 337) are as follows: delusions of guilt (e.g., feeling guilty over fire illness of a loved one), delusions of deserved punishment, delusions of world or personal destrucfion, somafic delusions, or delusions of poverty (e.g., being bankrupt). If firere are accompanying hallucinations, firey are usually transient and are often of voices berafing fire individual. If fire hallucinations are not associated wifir fire depressive mood direcfiy, firey may have persecutory firemes, delusions of firought insertions, delusions of firought broadcasfing, and delusions firat one’s firought are not your own. Major depression wifir psychofic symptoms has a poorer prognosis firan eifirer Dysfirymia or Major Depression. Characteristics of Mofirors wifir Clinical” Significant Depression According to a survey of women in fire United States, 8% of mofirors are clinically depressed at any given fime (Weissman, Gammon, John, 8 Merikangas, 1987; Weissman 8 Warner, 1997), firus putfing fireir children at risk for developing behavior problems (Dodge, 1990; Downey 8 Coyne, 1990; Rutter, 1995). In her studies of depressed mofirers, Campbell (1995) found firat depressed mofirers often show a combination of fire following qualifies: anger, hosfile tone of voice, flat or negafive affect, unemofional tone of voice, wifirdrawal, apafiry, anxiety, displeasure, intrusiveness, 32 disapproval of children, and evidence of behavioral disturbance. Also, Campbell (1995) described addifional characterisfics of depressed mofirers include negafive worldview, less engagement wifir ofirers, and difficulty in engaging in difficult and confiictual conversafions. In contrast, non- depressed mofirers had fire following qualifies: kind tone of voice, warm, posifive affect, ability to structure fire environment for fire child, ability to read fire child’s cues correcfiy, flexibility, and enjoyment of fireir interacfion wlfir fireir child (Campbell, 1995). lnteracfion Between Depressed Mofirers and Their Children Dix (1991) argues convincingly firat emofion is fire integrafiva force firat unites context (Darling 8 Steinberg, 1993) and content for parent as firey manage fire responsibilifies of child rearing, and firerefore, according to Dix (1991 ), parenfing is primarily an affecfive experience. Difficulfios wlfir affect, including depression, firerefore, would be expected to influence fire parent-child relafionship. Studies of mofirer-child interacfions support firis hypofiresis. Depressed mofirers, for example, often have deficit in fire kinds of skills firat contribute to posifive interacfion wifir fireir children (Hops, 1995). In mofirer-child interacfions, depressed mofirers are less posifive and more negafive (Campbell, Cohn, 8 Meyers, 1995), less sensifive in understanding and responding to fireir children’s needs, and less comfortable wifir fireir child (T efi, Gelfand, Messinger, 8 Isabella, 1995). Also, when parficipafing in a structured task wifir firelr child, depressed mofirers do less teaching 33 and less joint problem solving firan do fireir non-depressed counterpart (Goldsmifir 8 Rogoff, 1995; Goldsmifir 8 Rogoff, 1997). These parenfing difficulfies are closely associated wifir children who have troubled behavior. Characterisfig of Children of Depressed Mothers The qualifies firat depressed mofirers bring to fireir interacfions wifir firelr children put fireir children at significanfiy higher risk for developing psychopafirology compared to children of non-depressed mofirers. - Billings and Moos (1983, 1986) found firat children of depressed parent displayed more physical, psychological, and behavior problems firan did children of non-depressed parent, and fire more risk factors, such as fewer economic and social resources, fire more child behavior problems. Two such problems common for children of depressed mofirers are externalizing and internalizing behavior difficulfies (Baker 8 Heller, 1996; Campbell, 1995; Downey 8 Coyne, 1990; Goodman 8 Gofiib, 1999; Greenberg, Lengua, Coie, 8 Pinderhughes, 1999). The focus of firis study is on externalizing behavior problems, alfirough it is acknowledged firat often children of depressed parent display bofir externalizing (behavior firat is focused outward) and internalizing (behavior firat is focused inward) behavior problems. Extemalizing Behavior Problems. The goal of externalizing behavior (i.e., opposifional and aggressive behavior) for fire young child may be described as fire expression of emofion (eifirer posifive or negafive) andIor 34 fire desire to attain social goals (i.e., obtaining a desired toy or playing wifir a specific child or children in a social setting). Before furfirer discussing externalizing behavior, however, it is important to acknowledge firat young children often display opposifionality and aggression in fireir behavior as a normal funcfion of fire developmental process (Davies, 1999). The peak of aggressive behavior in young children occurs between fire ages of 2 and 4; after fire acquisifion of language, however, fire physically aggressive and opposifional behavior often seen in young children gives way to fire use of language as a more appropriate and producfive way to express feelings and desires and to make request (Davies, 1999). Extemalizing behaviors are considered dimensional, firat is firey have more firan one component, and firese component may exist on a confinuum from mild to very severe. One classificafion system for externalizing behavior as discussed by Stonnshak, Bierman, and colleagues (1998) describes four types of externalizing behaviors in young children. They are 1) opposifional, 2) oppositional/aggressive, 3) hyperacfivelattenfive, and 4) hyperacfivelinattenfive and opposifionallaggressive. Each of firese types may have different efiologies and different manifestafions for home and school behavior. In describing fire dimensions of externalizing behavior, fire trajectory from opposifional to more aggressive behavior represent a move to more difficult and more intense behavior. This change in intensity may be associated wifir more 35 problemafic parent-child interacfions andIor more exposure to negafive circumstances and event (White, 1995). For some young children, however, behavior firat is externalizing confinues into early childhood, and, in fact, may become a stable component of fire behavioral repertoire (Campbell, 1995; Caspi et al., 1997). Severe behavior difficulfies in early childhood may predispose fire child to difficulfies in adapfing to fire school environment — bofir academically and socially. A child manifesfing externalizing behaviors in fire classroom is less likely to focus on acquisifion of skills and knowledge in fire classroom, firus compromising school performance (Brigman, Lane, Switer, Lane, 8 Lawrence, 1999; Campbell, 1998; Campbell et al., 1991a, 1991 b; Campbell 8 Pierce, 1996; Coolahan, Fantuzzo, Mendez, 8 McDermott, 2000). In addifion, overly aggressive behavior in early childhood may indicate firat fire child has difficulfies engaging appropriately wifir peers - bofir entering groups already involved in acfivity, or once gaining admission, sustaining posifive and appropriate interacfion. These group skills are important for developmentally appropriate parficipafion in academic and peer acfivifies (Cummings, lannotfi, 8 Zahn-Waxler, 1989; Eisert, Walker, Severson, 8 Block, 1988; Kalpidou, Rofirbaum, 8 Rosen, 1998; Lambert, Weisz, 8 Thesiger, 1998; Quiggle, Garber, Pnak, 8 Dodge, 1992; Zahn-Waxler, Cole, Richardson, Friedman, Michel, 8 Boloud, 1994). Furfirerrnore, preschool measures of behavior problems and difficulfies wifir language development are among strong predictors of 36 stable and pervasive anfisocial behavior in late childhood and mid- adolescence (Loeber, 1991; Loeber 8 Dishion, 1983; Loeber 8 Stoufiramer- Loeber, 1998; Moffitt, 1990). The importance of understanding fire contributors to fire development, emergence, maintenance, and decline of externalizing behavior is important for fire future of young children who may be at risk for it manifestafion in fireir lives. 37 QflLlé The overall goal of fire current study was to better understand how maternal and family funcfioning and economic condifions influence fire cognifive and emofional development of young Jamaican children. The parficular maternal and family variables were maternal psychopafirology, family cohesion, and socioeconomic status; fire child outcome variables were behavior problems, verbal ability, and puzzle-solving skill. Quesfions of Interest Maternal Psychogfirolpgy Maternal psychopafirology (dysfirymia and major depression wifir psychotic features) is a known risk factor for troubled child outcome and is often associated wifir child behavior problems and delayed verbal ability. Mofirers who are dysfirymic or have major depression wifir psychofic features also have difficulty posifively engaging and sustaining pleasurable interacfions wifir fireir child. Because firis is fire first fime fire influence of firese factors on child outcome has been examined in a Jamaican population, no predicfions were made about possible differences in fire effect of firese two kinds of maternal psychopafirologies. Predicfion 1. For young Jamaican children, maternal dysfirymia and maternal major depression wifir psychofic features will be associated wifir more emofional and social immaturity, less verbal ability, less puzzle- solving skill, and less family cohesion. 38 Predicfion 2. For young Jamaican children, maternal psychopafirology will mediate fire relafion between SES and all child outcome measures. Family Variables As discussed previously, fire Jamaican family plays an especially vital role in fire rearing of children. It was firerefore, deemed to be important to consider fire qualifies of fire family and how firey influenced child outcome and maternal psychopafirology. For a well-funcfioning family, one such quality is cohesion. Based on evidence about fire influence of family cohesion on child outcome; fire ofirer about family cohesion as a mediator for fire relafion between SES and child outcome. Predicfion 3. For young Jamaican children, fire less family cohesion, fire worse fire child outcome, firat is fire greater number of behavior problems, fire less verbal ability, and less developed puzzle-solving skills. Predicfion 4. For young Jamaican children, family cohesion will mediate fire effect between SES and all child outcome measures. Socioeconomic Status For fire current smdy, fire influence of SES was considered in two ways. The first way was to consider SES as a variable firat direcfiy influences all ofirer variables in fire model (see Fig. 1). The second way was to consider SES influencing only maternal psychopafirology and firen 39 to covary SES wifir ofirer variables as suggested by the modificafion indices suggested by analysis. Predicfion 5. For young Jamaican children, lower family SES will be associated wifir poorer performance on all child outcome measures, more maternal psychopafirology, and lower family cohesion. 40 METHOD Parficignt The participant were 151 mothers between fire ages of 20and 51 (M = 30.26, SD = 7.10) and 151 children between fire ages of 3 to 6 (M = 5.04, SD = .94) who were recruited as part of a larger study (N =521 mofirers and 521 children) examining fire effect of iron deficiency anemia on fire cognifive and behavior development of Jamaican pro-school children (Samms-Vaughn, 1998a, Samms-Vaughn, 1998b). Parficipant were recruited from six Basic Schools (pro-schools and kindergartens), representafive of a typical populafion of urban Jamaican children. The following firree major exclusion criteria were used: 1) children wifir factors known or suspected to be associated wifir developmental delay such as low birfir weight (< 2,500gms), lead poisoning, malnutrifion, previous idenfified development delay; 2) children wifir laboratory findings firat will complicate interpretafions of result; and 3) efirical grounds (e.g., children whose hemoglobin count is less firan 8gldl will require immediate troatrnent). Selecfion of Iron Study Sample Parent of children from identified basic schools were interviewed to idenfify firose who do not meet study criteria on historical grounds. Following venepuncture and laboratory invesfigafion, firose children who did not meet hematological criteria were idenfified and excluded from further study. The study children were firose who met bofir historical and 41 laboratory criteria. Based on a 42% prevalence of iron deficiency anemia in Jamaican 2-4 year olds, twice fire number of children required for fire study sample underwent laboratory invesfigafion to idenfify an appropriate sample of study children. Table 1 displays fire demographic inforrnafion for fire 151 study children and fireir mofirers. Table 1: Descripfive Stafisfics N Mean SD. Minimum Maximum Mofirer Mofirer’s Age 151 30.87 6.44 19.0 51.0 Mofirer’s Educafion 151 3.77 0.69 1.0 6.0 Child Child’s Age 151 5.05 0.19 3.34 6.5 Family Socioeconomic Status 151 15.92 2.53 6.0 19.0 Dempgraphic lnfonnafion For fire sample of parficipant in fire larger study, fire following demographic inforrnafion was available. Table 1 present demographic characterisfics of fire study sample. Household Structure Ninety-firree percent of fire children lived wifir fireir biological mofirers. Where biological mofirers were absent, fire mofirer figure was represented by stepmofirers, adopfive mofirers, or grandmofirers. Grandmofirers, whefirer or not firey were mofirer figures, lived in 23% of fire homes. 42 Fafirers were present in only 50% of fire homes. In 15% of homes, firere was a fafirer figure, who commonly was a stepfafirer or grandfafirer, and, less commonly, an uncle. Fafirers and fafirer figures firus were present in 65% of fire homes. Aunt and uncles were present in as many as 41% of fire homes, wifir most homes including one or fire other. Child Rearing In keeping wifir fire matriarchal household structure just described, 87% of fire children were reared by firelr biological mofirer, 13% by a grandmofirer, 3% by a stepmofirer, and 3% by fire biological fafirer. In more firan 90% of cases, fire child had lived wifir fire current caretaker for most or all of fire child’s life. Only 6% of current caretakers reported living Iitfie or never wifir fire child in fire past. Living Condifions Eighty-five percent of fire homes had working bafirrooms. In 66%, fire bafirroom was in fire home, in 19%, It was outide fire home and shared wifir ofirer families. 15% of fire homes had no modern toilet, requiring inhabitant to use a pit Iatrine. Ninety-five percent of families had access to running water; fire rest used different mefirods of water catchment from a source. Of firose wifir running water, 66% had pipes in fire home, 14% had pipes in fire yards, and 17% had pipes outide fire yard. In fire last two cases, fire water supply was shared wifir ofirer families. 43 Ninety-five percent of fire families had access to electricity; fire rest used oil lamps or candlelight at night. Ninety-four percent used gas or electricity to cook and fire rest used coal or wool (4%) or kerosene (1%). Basic Schools Six Basic Schools wifirin the Kingston and St. Andrew region parficipated in fire current study. To facilitate transportafion to and from fire Dopartrnent of Child Healfir research center at fire University of West Indies, schools were chosen for fireir proximity (wifirin 2 miles) of fire center. Parficipafing schools were: 1) Water Commission Basic School, 2) Providence Basic School, 3) August Town Sevenfir Day Advenfist Basic School, 4) Mona Commons Basic School, 5) Hope Valley Experimental Infant School, and 6) Shady Grove Basic School. Alfirough all schools were classified as Basic Schools, firey had different administrafive structures. Two schools (Providence and August Town) were affiliated wifir churches and were located on church property. Two schools were community-based wifirout church affiliafion (Mona Commons and Shady Grove), one school was funded by a private organizafion (Water Commission), and one school was an Infant Department of a Primary School (Hope Valley). Basic Schools firroughout Jamaica chiefly serve lower middle or lower income families. Wifirin firls range, firere are sfill noficeable differences. August Town and Mona Commons Basic Schools are in communifies wifir significant social and economic deprivafion and primarily serve fire local community. Water Commission, Providence, Hope Valley, and Shady Grove Basic Schools serve families wifir more economic resources. Family Measures Socioeconomic Status Measures of socioeconomic status (SES), such as income earned and current occupafion firat are commonly used in Norfir America do not accurately measure SES in developing countries, including Jamaica (Leo- Rhynie 8 Hamilton, 1993; Samms-Vaughn, 1998a,b). There is, however, no accepted standard for assessing SES in developing countries. In Jamaica, one mefirod firought to more accurately reflect SES calculates fire number of appliances and material goods in fire home (e.g., number and quality of toilet facilifies, fire place where water is obtained, quality of indoor Iighfing, type of stove). The higher fire number and quality of appliances and material goods, fire higher fire SES score. Rose (1998) found firat SES scores computed in firis way predicted severity of psychopafirology (for bofir men and women) as measured on fire Global Severity Index of fire Brief Symptom Inventory (Dorogafis, 1992; Derogafis, 1993) as accurately as did ofirer measures of SES in ofirer populafions, wifir lower scores being related to higher psychopafirology. Family Cohesion To assess family cohesion, The Family Adaptability and Cohesion Environment Scales-ll (herein called FACES)(OIson 8 Russell, 1980; Olson 45 et al., 1979) was used. The instrument present 30 descripfions of behaviors and atfitudes about families (e.g., family does firings together; children have a say in fireir own discipline; family shares responsibilifies; family spends free fime togefirer), which fire mofirers were asked to rate on a 3-point scale as firey apply to firemselves and fireir own family (0 = no, 1 = somefimes, 2 I always). Confinnatory factor analyses revealed firat fire original scale structure (Olson et al., 1979) did not replicate in fire Jamaican sample (Lambert, Samms-Vaughn, Lyubansky, Podolski, Hannah, McCaslin, 8 Rowan, 1999b). An exploratory factor analysis (EFA), firerefore, was performed to determine item loadings on fire Jamaican factors. An analysis using principal axis factoring analysis was chosen because fire purpose was to find fire scale structures firat reflect fire Jamaican parficipant’ responses (Floyd 8 Widaman, 1995). Retenfion of factors, based on an eigenvalue 2 1, resulted in two factors, which firen were rotted wifir an oblique promax solufion. Items wifir loadings of z .30 on each factor were deemed as loading on fire respecfive dimension. The two factors derived from firese procedures were labeled Family Cohesion and Adaptability (herein called Family Cohesion) and Diminished Family Cohesion and Adaptability. Factors, factor loadings, and alphas are listed in Table 2. 46 Table 2: Faces-ll Dimensions Fa r1: Famll C h ion and Ada t ll Item Item Loadings Descrlpfions Alpha = .7940 7 Family does firings togefirer 0.633 13 Family consult about acfivifies 0.577 21 Family members go along wifir ofirers 0.528 11 Family members know other’s close friends 0.496 14 Family members say what firey want 0.491 22 Share responsibilifies 0.441 23 Spends free fime togefirer 0.430 30 Share hobbies 0.425 27 Family approves of ofirer’s friends 0.415 8 Family discusses problems 0.375 5 Family gafirers togefirer in same room 0.374 6 Children have say in fireir discipline 0.343 20 Tries new ways of dealing wlfir problems 0.341 10 Shift household chores 0.332 4 Family members have input in decisions 0.327 Eigenvalue 3.821 Factor 2: Family Non-Cohesiveness and Non-Adaptbill_ty Item Item Descripfions Loadings Alpha = .7940 15 Difficulty doing firings togefirer 0.676 25 Family members avoid each ofirer at home 0.587 19 Feel closer to non-family members 0.581 29 Do firings in twos not as whole family 0.516 24 Difficult to get rules changed in family 0.501 12 Hard to know rules changed in family 0.428 1 Family supporfive -0.399 3 Easier to tlk outide family 0.347 Eigenvalue 3.286 47 Maternal Measures Matrnal Psychogfirology Brief Symmm Inventory. The Brief Symptom Inventory (BSI) was used to measure maternal psychopafirology. The BSI, which is currenfiy widely used for bofir research and clinical assessment, is a self-report symptom inventory wifir 53 itms. Each item is rated on a 5-point scale from 0 to 4, where 0 indicates no distress and 4 indicates extreme distress. The BSI has a totl of nine symptom dimensions: Somaficizafion, Obsessive-Compulsive, Interpersonal Sensifivity, Depression, Anxiety, Phobic Anxiety, Paranoid ldeafion, Psychofism, and Hosfility. The BSI was chosen for fire assessment of maternal psychopafirology for firree reasons. First, it is quick and easy to administer, a necessary criterion for dat collecfion. Second, it has solid psychometric properfies. The internal consistency reliability for fire nine factors has been found to range from .71 for Psychoficism to .85 for Depression in a sample of 719 psychiatric pafient (Derogafis, 1993). Third, it is easily understood by Jamaicans and, firerefore, could be given wifirout revision (Rose, 1998). When Confirmatory Factor Analysis (CFA) was performed on fire BSI, fire derived indices indicated firat fire factor structure did not replicate for fire Jamaican sample (Lambert et al., 1999). Exploratory Factor Analysis (principal component analysis), firerefore, was performed to idenfify fire factor structure for Jamaicans. Six factors were derived: 1) Somafic 48 Complaint; 2) Paranoia; 3) Hosfility; 4) Mild but chronic Depression or Dysfirymia; 5) Major Depression wifir Psychofic Features; and 6) Agoraphobia wifir Panic Disorder. (For furfirer detils of fire item loadings, see Lambert et al., 1999). As noted earlier, alfirough depression is often considered to be a homogeneous disorder, research on fire component of maternal psychopafirology suggest firat is actually heterogeneous (Clayton, 1998). Evidence for firis heterogeneity also was revealed in fire EPA for fire Jamaican sample, which discovered two disfinct types of depression: Mild but Chronic Depression (herein called dysfirymia) (Scale 4) and Major Depression wifir Psychofic Features (herein called depression)(Scale 5). Therefore, for fire current study bofir depression scales and Depression were chosen for inclusion in fire analyses of pafirs leading to child outcome, wifir a separat analysis being performed for each scale. Factor loadings for Scales 4 and 5 are in Table 3. Child Measures Child Behavior Child behavior funcfioning was measured by fire Connors Parent Rafing Scale (Goyette, Connors, 8 Ulrich, 1978). The version used in fire current study included 48 sttement (e.g., picks at firings, sassy to grown- ups) firat fire mofirer was instructed to rate, on a 3-point scale, for how well firey described her own child (0 = never, 1 = a litfie, 2 = a whole lot). Confirmatory factor analyses revealed firat fire original scale structures did 49 Table 3: BSI Dimensions for Depression Dimension 4: Mild Depression or Dsflmia Item Item Descripfions Loadings Alpha = .7892 14 Feel lonely when wifir ofirers 0.72 15 Feeling blocked in getfing firings done 0.54 16 Feeling lonely 0.79 17 Feeling sad 0.79 18 No interest in firings 0.50 44 Never feeling close to ofirers 0.40 Eigenvalue 7.53 Dimension 5: Major Depression wlfir Psychofic Features Item Item Doscripfions Loadings Alpha = .8267 3 Idea firat ofirers can control firought 0.41 9 Thought of ending own life 0.56 22 Feeling inferior to ofirers 0.39 34 Idea firat committed sins should be punished for 0.47 35 Hopeless about future 0.60 45 Spells of terror and panic 0.39 50 Feeling worfirless 0.67 52 Feelings of guilt 0.42 53 Idea firat somefiring is wrong wifir mind 0.65 Eigenvalue 8.16 50 not replicate in fire Jamaican sample (Lambert et al., 1999). An exploratory factor analysis (EFA), firerefore, was performed to determine item loadings on fire Jamaican factors. Again, principle factoring axis analysis was chosen in order to find fire scale structures firat reflect fire Jamaican parficipant’ responses (Floyd 8 Widaman, 1995). Retenfion of factors, again based on an eigenvalue 2 1, resulted in firree factors. As before, fire factors were rotted wifir an oblique promax solufion. Items wifir loadings of > .30 on each factor were deemed as loading on fire respecfive dimension. The firree factors derived from firis procedure were: 1) Child Emofion, and Social Immaturity 2) Somafic Complaint; and 3) Moodiness. The scale used in fire current study was fire Child Emofion and Social Immaturity Scale (herein called Child Behavior Problems), a mix of behavior, emofion, and social difficulfies typical of young children. For example, young children are impulsive, cry easily, and often disobedient (T remblay, 1999). Factor component and factor loadings for fire Connors appear in Table 4. Verbal Ability and Puzzle-Solving McCarfiry Scales of Children’s Abillfies. The McCarfiry Scales of Children’s Abilifies (Kaufman 8 Kaufman, 1977) are a well stndardized and psychometrically sound measure of fire cognifive abilifies of children from 2.5 to 8.5 years of age (Satfier, 1992). They are administered by examiners to individuals, and depending on fire child’s age, tko 45 to 60 minutes to administer. According to Satfier (1992), fire scales are appropriate for 51 Table 4: Connors Factors Item Item Descriptions Loadings Factor 1: Social Immatumy Alpha = 0.8251 14 Destrucfive 0.683 4 Impulsive 0.606 15 Tells lies 0.585 9 Daydreams 0.579 2 Is sassy to grown-ups 0.534 17 More trouble than ofirers 0.489 29 Cruel 0.451 37 Easily frustrated 0.448 38 Disturbs other children 0.446 5 Want to run things 0.437 10 Has difficulty Ieaming 0.419 27 Bullies others 0.373 23 Disobedient 0.352 30 Childish 0.343 16 Shy 0.337 3 Has problems wifir friends 0.332 24 Worries more than others 0.331 28 Engages in repefitive acfivifies 0.306 35 Fight constnfiy 0.302 7 Crles easily 0.302 Eigenvalue 3.821 Factor 2: Somafic Complaint Alpha I 0.6697 41 Stomach aches 0.627 39 Unhappy 0.479 44 Vomiting 0.475 47 Let’s self be pushed around by others 0.443 18 Speaks differenfiy 0.400 48 Bowel problems 0.396 45 Feels cheated 0.387 43 Other aches 0.354 42 Sleep problems 0.329 Eigpnvplue 3.784 Factor 3: Moodiness Alpha = .5019 11 Squirrny 0.401 33 Mood changes often 0.387 13 Always up and on the go 0.381 21 Pout and sulks 0.370 36 Doesn’t get along with siblings 0.333 26 Feelings easily hurt 0.319 Eigenvalue 2.851 52 children wifir Ieaming problems or ofirer special needs. They reveal a general measure of intellectual funcfioning called fire General Cognifive Index (GCI), which consist of fire following Scales: Verbal, Perceptual- Perfonnance, Quanfitfive, Memory, and Motor. For fire current study, Verbal Ability (full scale) and Puzzle-Solving (a subtest of fire Perceptual Performance Scale) were used as measures of verbal ability and non-verbal ability, respecfively. Verbal ability, which is highly correlated wifir success in scth (T remblay, 1999), assesses fire ability to understnd and process verbal infonnafion and to express drought in words. It consist of six subscales: pictorial memory, word knowledge, verbal memory, verbal fluency, and opposite analogies. The subscales measure such capacifies as short-term memory, attenfion, verbal expression, verbal comprehension, logical classificafion, and verbal reasoning and is highly correlated wifir success in school (T remblay, 1999). Puzzle-solving, a skill often called upon in programs in early childhood educafion, tps fire child’s abilifies in visual percepfion, non- verbal reasoning, visual-motor coordinafion, and spafial relafions, among ofirers. Psychometric Progrfies Stndardizafion. Stndardizafion of fire McCarfiry Scales involved 1,032 children between fire ages of 2.5 and 8.5. The following variables were included in strafificafion: age, sex, race, geographic region, fafirer’s occupafion, and urban-rural residence. 53 The McCarfiry Scales reveal five indices firat are reported as stndard scores, wifir M = 50 and SD = 10. The overall General Cognifive Index (GCI) has a M = 100 and SD = 16 and gives an approximafion of fire child’s ability to use acquired knowledge and to adapt firat knowledge to new tsks. Reliabilifies for each of fire scales are reported for fire 10 different age groups included in fire stndardization sample (Satfier, 1992). The average split-half reliability is for fire General Cognifive Index (rm, =.93)and ranges from .79 to .88 for fire ofirer five scales. The McCarfiry Scales have acceptble concurrent validity wifir fire Stnford Binet—Form L-M, WISC, WISC-R, WPPSI, K-ABC, and Slosson Intelligence Test used as criteria (Satfier, 1992, p. 298, alfirough for learning disabled children, fire concurrent validity is not as acceptble (Satfier, 1992,p.298L Scores on fire McCarfiry Scales are significanfiy correlated wifir scores on such stndard achievement test as fire Metropolitn Achievement Test, Peabody Individual Assessment Test, and fire Wide- Range Achievement Test, indicafing safisfactory predlcfive validity (Satfier, 1992,p.298L 54 RESULTS Dat Analygis Only observed variables were used in fire model. Pafir modeling is appropriate for analyzing manifest variables because it includes fire measurement error of all endogenous variables in fire analysis (Klem, 1995). Table 5 present the bivariate correlafions of fire variables analyzed in fire model. Amos 4 (Arbuckle 8 Wofirko, 1999) was used to obtin fire maximum likelihood esfimates of fire model coefficient. Model Fit. The adequacy of fit for fire pafir models was determined by considering fire following indices in combinafion: ChiSquare (x2) stfisfic, Goodness of Fit Index (GFI), Comparafive Fit Index (CPI), and Root-Mean-Square Error of Approximafion (RMSEA)(for informafion about firese indices, see Hu and Benfior, 1995). In general, a good-fitfing model is one wifir a non-significant Chi-Square, a GFI and CFI of .90 or larger, and a RMSEA of .05 or less. Pafir Analflis The first predicfion was firat socioeconomic sttus (herein called SES) would direcfiy predict child outcome including child behavior Immaturity, emofion immaturity, and social immaturity (hereafter called “child behavior problems”), child verbal ability, and child puzzle-solving skill. The second predicfion, which derived from fire first, was firat fire direct relafion between SES and child outcome would be mediated by 55 Table 5: Bivariate Correlafions for all Variables in Models 9'99.” 1 2 Dysthymia 1.00 Depression .618‘ 1.00 Cohesion -.223*" — SES ~235” -.355“ Behavior —- —— Problems Verbal .— Ability Solving 1 .00 .185‘ -.223“ 1 .00 —— 1.00 .203” — 1 .00 .247“ —— .419” 1.00 *p < .05 *‘p > .01 56 maternal psychopafirology and family cohesion. SES was also expected to be negafively related to maternal psychopafirology and posifively related to family cohesion. Psychopafirology, in turn, was expected to be posifively related to child behavior problems and negafively related to verbal ability and puzzle-solving. As for cohesion, it was expected to be negafively related to child behavior problems, and posifively related to verbal ability and puzzle-solving. Result for Models 1 and 2: Dflfirymia Models Models 1 and 2, fire first two pafir models to be considered, examined fire relafion of socioeconomic sttus, maternal dysfirymia, and family cohesion to child outcome. The result are shown in Figures 2 and 3. Model Fit. Bofir models parfially supported fire outcome predicfions, firat is, had an overall acceptble fit to fire dat. For Model 1 fire chi-square was not significant [12(2), N = 151)= 2.511, p = 0.285], fire GFI was 0.994, fire CFI was 0.992, and fire RMSEA was .055. Because bofir models fit fire dat, fire chi-square difference test was conducted to determine whefirer one model made a better fit firan fire ofirer. The result revealed firat bofir models fit fire dat equally well [xZDelt(2) = 3.321, N.S.]. M16211 Socioeconomic Sttus. The first predicfion - firat SES would predict child behavior problems, verbal ability, and puzzle-solving skills -- was not fully supported in Model 1. SES predicted neifirer child behavior problems 57 , .06 Maternal 3'7" "24 Dysthymia .09 ”J Child Behavior ,:\--"‘~~ Problems ’4 r ” " ’ p . r.~~ \ ~“ 323 ..... , . 04 ‘~ \ I §‘~ ~~~~ \ u‘- \\ ‘7. . \ .09 Family Cohesion ...... '07 ............ Puzzle- “““ N Solving Skill P < .05* P < .01“ "—'> Indicates significant pafir ......... > Indicates non-significant pafir Figure 2. Model 1: Influence of SES, Matrnal Dysfirymia, and Family Cohesion on Child Outcome 58 Maternal J9” Dysfirymia ~ 08 \“ . ‘\ \ I \ Child \ ‘ “ Bah ' \ aVIor \ \ \ Problems \ ‘s \\ \\ \ \ \ \ \ -.28** x “- \\ ‘\\ SES \ ‘ .08 t Family L/ Cohesion ...... —i '00 ............ Puzzle- “““ 5 Solving Skill P < .05* P < .01” ——§ Indicats significant pafir --------- 5 Indicates non-significant pafil Figure 3. Model 2: Influence of SES, Maternal Dysfirymia, and Family Cohesion, and Child Outcome 59 nor verbal ability, meaning firat their predicted medlafion by maternal dysfirymia and family cohesion also was not supported. SES, however, did predict puzzle-solving alfirough puzzle-solving, contrary to predicfion, was not mediated by dysfirymia or family cohesion. The result also showed fire following direct relafions to be significant: a) negafivo relafion between SES and maternal dysfirymia (lower levels of SES predicted higher levels of dysfirymia); b) posifive relafion between maternal dysfirymia and child behavior problems (fire lower dysfirymia, fire fewer number of problems); c) negafive relafion between dysfirymia and family cohesion (fire lower fire level of dysfirymia, fire higher fire cohesion); d) and negafive relafion between family cohesion and child behavior problems (fire lower fire cohesion, fire higher fire number of problems). Alfirough SES did not direcfiy predict child behavior problems, it was involved in two indirect pafirs: a) from SES to dysfirymia and from dysfirymia to behavior problems and b) from SES to dysfirymia, from dysfirymia to cohesion, and from cohesion to behavior problems. Dypfirymia. Matrnal dysfirymia was negafively and significanfiy related to family cohesion (fire lowerthe dysfirymia, fire higher fire cohesion) and posifively and significanfiy related to child behavior problems (fire higher fire dysfirymia, fire higher fire number of problems). Famiu Cohesion. Family cohesion was direcfiy and inversely relafion to child behavior problems (fire lower fire cohesion, fire higher fire number of problems). 60 Mediafion. As previously stted, fire original mediafion hypofiresis was not supported. The result, however, revealed a parfial mediafion role for cohesion and for fire relafion between dysfilymia and child behavior problems such firat fire lower fire dysfirymia, fire higher fire cohesion, and fire higher fire cohesion, fire lower fire number of problems. Variance. Model 1 accounted for 35% of fire variance, divided as follows: 20% for child outcome (9% for behavior problems, 4% for verbal ability, and 7% for puzzle-solving), 6% for dysfirymia, and 9% for family cohesion. M Socioeconomic Sttus. In Model 2, SES predicted only dysfirymia and covaried wifir child verbal ability and puzzle-solving skill. The following direct pafirs were significant: a) negafive relafion between SES and dysfirymia (lower levels of SES predicted higher levels of dysfirymia; b) posifive relation between dysfirymia and child behavior problems (fire lower fire dysfirymia, fire lower fire number of problems); c) negafive relafion between dysfirymia and cohesion (fire lower fire level of dysfirymia, fire higher fire cohesion); and d) negafive relafion between family cohesion and child behavior problems (fire lower fire cohesion, fire higher fire number of problems). Again, alfirough SES did not direcfiy predict child behavior problems, it was involved in two indirect pafirs: a) from SES to dysfirymia 61 and from dysfirymia to behavior problems; and b) from SES to dysfirymia, from dysfirymia to cohesion, and from cohesion to behavior problems. Qfifirymia. Maternal dysthymia was negafively and significanfiy related to family cohesion (fire lower fire dysfilymia, fire higher fire cohesion) and posifively and significanfiy related to child behavior problems (fire higher fire dysfirymia, fire higher fire number of problems). Family Cohesion. Family cohesion was direcfiy and inversely related to child behavior problems (file lower fire cohesion, fire higher fire number of problems). Mediafion. As previously sttd, fire original mediafion hypofiresis was not supported. The result, however, revealed parfial modiafion, namely, firat cohesion parfially mediated fire relafion between maternal dysfirymia and child behavior problems such firat fire lower fire dysfirymia, fire higher fire cohesion, and fire higher fire cohesion, fire lower fire number of problems. This is fire same parfial mediafion process firat was revealed in Model 1. Variance. Model 2 accounted for 22% of fire variance divided as follows: 8% for child outcome (all for behavior problems), 8% for cohesion, and 6% for maternal dysfirymia. In sum, models 1 and 2 accounted for 26% and 22% of fire variance, respecfively, leaving a substnfial amount unaccounted for in bofir models. 62 Result for Models 3 and 4: Major Depression wifir Psychofic Features Models Models 3 and 4, fire Major Depression wifir Psychofic Features Models (herein called fire Depression Model), examined fire relafion of SES, maternal depression, and family cohesion on child outcome. Result of fire pafir analysis for firese models are shown in Figures 4 and 5. Model Fit. As was fire case for models 1 and 2, bofir Models 3 and 4 parfially supported fire outcome predicfions, firat is, bofir had an overall acceptble fit to fire dat. For Model 3 fire chi-square was not significant [x2(2), _N_ s 151) = 2.091, p = 0.352], fire GFI was 0.995, fire CFI was 0.999, and fire RMSEA was 0.017. Likewise, for Model 4 fire chi-square was not significant [x2(4), N = 151) = 6.756, p = 0.116], fire GFI was .985, fire CFI was .976, and fire RMSEA was .048. Because bofir models fit fire dat, fire chi- square difference test was conducted to determine whefirer one made a better fit firan fire ofirer. The result revealed firat bofir models fit fire dat equally well [xZDelt(3) = 4.665, N.S.]. Result of fire pafir analysis for firese two models can be found in Figures 4 and 5. M Socioeconomic sttus. For Model 3, fire first predicfion was firat SES would predict child behavior problems, verbal ability and puzzle-solving skills. The predicfion was parfiy supported. SES did not predict child behavior problems or verbal ability, which meant firat fireir predicted mediafion by maternal depression and family cohesion also was not 63 .13 Major Depression ~~~~~~~ With Psychofic “““““““ 06 Features ““““““ * ' Child -.35** : -.22** Behavior E ,,,,,,,,,, ' Problems 1 :2? """" I ,,,,,, \ .- —' ’ " ’ \ pppp l \ I SE s l. ‘ ~ ~ - j ‘\‘ 08 i .......... ‘ Verbal . . ..\ ...... \ 5 Ability \ E -.20** 3‘“? \ ,v " \\ \\ _ .04 fi ,,,,,, \ \ , v ‘\ “ Family """" x ‘ v" .26“ \ Cohesion ...... ~07 .......... Puzzle- “““ Solving Skill P < .05” P < .01” —'> Indicates significant pafir ......... 8 Indicates non-significant pafir Figure 4. Model 3: Influence of SES, Major Depression wlfir Psychofic Features, and Family Cohesion on Child Outcome Major Depression wifir Psychofic ‘xx "5 Symptoms “‘x- ‘ ‘ ‘‘‘‘‘ Child ‘ Behavior -.35** -.26** Problems SES \\ ”22” ’07 Verbal Ability .02 ’’’’’’’’’ Family ,,,,,,, 00 Cohesion ....... .4 - ............ Puzzle- “““ 5 Solving Skill P < .05* ——> Indicates significant path --------- § Indicates non-significant path Figure 5. Model 4: Influence of SES, Major Depression wifir Psychofic Features, and Family Cohesion on Child Outcome 65 supported. SES, however, did predict puzzle-solving, alfirough puzzle- solving skill was not mediated by depression or family cohesion. In Model 3, lower SES was negafively and significanfiy related to levels of maternal depression (fire lower fire SES, fire higher fire depression) and posifively and significanfiy related to child puzzle-solving skills (fire lower fire SES, fire lower fire skill). Alfirough SES did not direcfiy predict child behavior problems, fire model did reveal one indirect pafir to child outcome: from SES to maternal depression and from depression to verbal ability. Depression. Maternal depression was negafively related to SES (fire higher fire depression, fire lower fire SES) and negafively related to verbal ability (fire higher fire depression, fire lower fire verbal ability). Contrary to predicfion, depression was not related, eifirer direcfiy or indirectly, to child behavior problems. Family Cohesion. Family cohesion was found to be significanfiy related to child behavior problems but, contrary to predicfion, was not related, eifirer direcfiy or indirecfiy, to maternal depression. Variance in Model 3. Model 3 accounted for 38% of the variance, divided as follows: 21% for child outcome (6% for behavior problems, 8% for verbal ability, 7% for puzzle-solving skill, 13% for maternal depression, and 4% for family cohesion. 66 ML! Socioeconomic Sttus. In Model 4, SES predicted only depression and covaried wifir child verbal ability and puzzle-solving skill. Only one direct pafir and one indirect pafir were significant. The direct pafir was a negafive relafion between SES and depression (lower levels of SES predicted higher levels of depression). The indirect pafir was from SES to depression and from depression to verbal ability. Depression. Depression was posifively related to child verbal ability, but contrary to predicfion, depression was not related to family cohesion, eifirer direcfiy or indirecfiy. ‘ Family Cohesion. Family cohesion was direcfiy and inversely related to child behavior problems (fire lower fire cohesion, fire more fire behavior problems), fire same pattern as found in previous models. Mediafion. As previously stted, fire original mediafion hypofiresis was not supported, and firere were no ofirer mediafion pafirs in firis model. Variance. Model 4 accounted for 25% of fire variance divided as follows: 12% for child outcome (5% for behavior problems, 7% for verbal ability) and 13% for maternal depression. 67 In sum, Models 3 and 4 accounted for 38% and 25% of fire variance, respecfively, leaving, as was fire case for Models 1 and 2, a substnfial amount unaccounted for. 68 DISCUSSION One specific concern idenfified by teachers of young children in Jamaica is fire emergence of serious behavior problems in fire classroom. Thus, firis has also become a serious issue for fire Jamaican Ministry of Educafion. This study, undertken from fire perspecfive of fire socioeconomic gradient (Keafing 8 Hartman, 1999) as a major influence on child outcome, examined family SES, maternal psychopafirology, and family cohesion as firree of many potenfial contributors to child outcome: emofion and social behavior, verbal ability, and puzzle-solving skill. The relafions firat emerged proved to be complex, and, as predicted, strongly influenced by SES. SESI Maternal PsychopafirolpgyI and Family Cohesion In studies focusing on children from low-income families and fireir performance in school setfings, SES is a salient predictor of outcome (Brooks-Gunn at al., 1999; Sfipek, 2001; Sfipak 8 Ryan, 1997). It also proved to be a strong contributor to child outcome in firis study. Low family SES, for example, dirocfiy predicted low puzzle-solving skill. One straightfonlvard explanafion is economic: low SES restrict fire family’s ability to purchase puzzles and, in addifion, many of fire Basic Schools, because of fireir own limited resources, do not have puzzles available elfirer. Thus, fire child’s opportunity to pracfice and master firat skill is not available. It may be, however, firat because Jamaican parent believe firat 69 school, even for young children, should focus on reading and wrifing firat firey do not invest limited resources on puzzles and similar materials. Lower SES is often associated wifir higher levels of maternal psychopafirology. In firis study, lower SES made a somewhat larger contribufion to major depression wifir psychofic features firan it did to maternal dysfirymia. These findings are not unique to Jamaica; in most countries firare is an astblished inverse relafion between SES and severity of mentl illness in fire populafion (Keafing 8 Hartman, 1999). Mofirers wifir low chronic levels of dysfirymia often have children wlfir behavior problems (Campbell, 1995; Lang, Field, Pickens, 8 Marfinaz, 1996; Stonnont, 1998). This may be because dysfirymia can restrict an individual’s desire to participate in daily acfivifies, and firus, might lower fire amount of fime fire mofirer spends wifir fire child, or mofirer-child intracfion may be more diracfive, demanding, or controlling, maternal factors associated wifir behavior problems in young children (Campbell, 1995). Higher levels of maternal dysfirymia are also reflected in lower levels of family cohesion. Because fire majority of Jamaican families are matriarchal, maternal dysfirymia may exert an influence over general family funcfioning. In firis study, dysfirymia influenced bofil fire family and fire child, by contribufing to lower levels of funcfioning for bofir. Contrary to predicfion, dysfirymia was not related to fire verbal ability of fire child, a strong relafion found in previous non-Jamaican studies, nor did it 70 contribute direcfiy to lower puzzle-solving skills; it major influence was behavioral only. Alfirough major depression wifir psychofic features is a more serious and debilitfing illness firan is dysfirymia, contrary to predicfion, major depression wifir psychofic features did not direcfiy predict child behavior problems, defined as emofion and social immaturity. At first glance, firis seems counter-intuifive. Perhaps it means firat mofirers wifir severe psychopafirology do not frequently interact wifir fireir children because . daily monitoring and care are tkan on by ofirer family members (Scott- McDonald, 1997). Thus, fire disturbed mofirer does not spend sufficient fime wifir fire child to influence it behavior, eifirar posifivaly or nagafively. High levels of major depression wifir psychofic features did, however, predict lower levels of child verbal ability. Because firere is less frequent mofirer-child interacfion, firere may be overall less conversafion and discussion wifil fire child, or fire conversafion and discussion may reflect fire cognifive deficit and distorfions firat are part of fire illness, firus manifesfing in lower verbal ability for fire child. Low family cohesion was direcfiy related to a high number of child behavior problems. This finding is similar to firose found in fire existing literature on family funcfioning and child outcome (Kupersmidt, J. B., Griesler, P. C., DeRosier, M. E., 8 Patterson, C. J., 1995; Mafirijssen, J.J. P., Koot, H. M., Verhulst, F. C., DaBruyn, E., 8 Oud, J. H., 1998; Sroufe, L. A. 8 Fleeson, J. 1988). 71 Thus, child behavior problems were independonfiy influenced by fire level of family cohesion and fire level of maternal dysfirymia, supporfing fire hypofiresis of fire importnco and influence of family on child outcome in Jamaican daily life. As also might be expected, family and maternal funcfioning were also influenced by fire level of available economic resources. The current study has some obvious limitfions. One such limitfion focuses on sample issues and fire ability to generalize from firis sample to all Jamaican children in early childhood programs. The sample, which was a recruited and not a random sample, came from one small secfion of fire island (Kingston) where SES is generally wifirin fire same low-end range (Grant, 1974). This raises issues of ecological validity and firus fire ability to generalize firase result across fire whole island of Jamaica is limited. Furfirer studies involving samples from a variety of early childhood programs serving fire full range of SES across Jamaica would need to be conducted to daterrnine if pafirs to child outcome, or child outcome itelf, remain fire same. There is some lndicafion firat firis may not be fire case; firat outcome is related to SES and to fire socializafion of fire children as to parentl expactfions for school parforrnance (Evans, 1989). A second Iimitfion concerns fire source of fire dat. All dat used to assess maternal psychopafirology, family cohesion, and child behavior problems came from fire mofilers, creafing a problem wifir mefirod variance (Pedhazur, 1973). To avoid such problems in fire future, infonnafion should 72 ideally come from mulfiple sources, including ofirer persons who are integral to fire household and from persons who interact wifir fire child outide fire home, including fire child’s teacher. A firird limitfion, related to fire second, is first fafirar or fafirer- figures were not included in fire study. It is known firat fafirers, however, play an important role, whefirer diracfiy or indiracfiy, in fire socializafion of a child (Brown, Newland, Anderson, 8 Chevannes, 1997; DeKIyan, M., Biernbaum, M. A., Greenberg, M. T., 8 Spelt, M. L., 1998). This problem could not be avoided in fire current study; no fafirers were included in fire sample. In future studies, every attempt should be made to include fafirels. While fire factor structures of fire measures were tested and new factors developed, one cannot be cartin firat fire measures held appropriate content and cultural validity for Jamaicans. Research should determine fire content structure for construct addressed in fire study. Because firis study was cross-secfional, what is not known is fire causal direcfion between fire variables. Lower SES may contribute to higher levels of psychopafirology; higher levels of psychopafirology may contribute to lower family and individual SES; or fire ralafions may be recursive. Our need for understnding causal direcfion for firese factors calls for future research to consider longitudinal designs (Campbell, 1991; Moffitt, 1990). Longitudinal design provides fire only true way to answer fire quesfions about davelopmentl process over fime. At firis point, very 73 few, if any, longitudinal studies have been done wifir Jamaican children and families. This should be a goal for further research. lmplicafions and Future Direcfions Jamaica has made a strong commitment to provide educafion for young children. For educators who teach young children, school readiness is an importnt issue and is complex in it dafinifions and attributes. As previously discussed, firrae factors, among many, idenfified as contribufing to a posifive experience in preschool are behavior - ragulafion, fire ability to use language to express feelings, ideas, and to ask quesfions, and puzzle-solving skills, a proxy for experience wifir manipulafivas and visual problem-solving. How do fire result of firis research inform fire firinking of fire Ministry of Educafion? As already stted, Jamaica is a developing country wlfir extremes of waalfir and poverty, high unemployment, and high inflafion and, for many, a declining stndard of living (Barfilow, 1997). The stressers, firerefora, on families and children are high and are likely to remain so for fire foreseeable future. In light of firese difficulfies, Jamaica’s decision to commit itelf strongly to oducafion is well-founded Inasmuch as an educated cifizenry is one of fire most effecfive ways to support social and economic development. The emergence of serious behavior problems among young children, a factor firat can seriously interfere wifir fireir learning, firarefore, is serious and needs to be addressed. Two avenues of explanafion, obviously 74 interrelated, are possible: contribufions of fire home may contribute to eifirer or hot fire child’s readiness to learn or fire child’s lack of readiness to funcfion in fire social context of school. Difficulfies wifir eifirer of firese factors have been associated wifir poor school adjustment (Lambert, M. C., Thesiger, C., Overly, K., 8 Knight, F., 1990). It may be, however, firat fire school curriculum and teachers are not suited to fire needs and capabilifies of fire child (Johnson 8 Brown, 1995; Morrison at al., 1998), and fire flexibility and skills needed by fire teachers and teacher-educators to modify fire curriculum may not be available. This may be because of administrafive or parentl expactfions or lack of proper training and support of teachers to meet fire axpactfions, or a combinafion of factors. From many tachers’ parspacfives, problem behaviors and fire skills necessary for school success begin at home. There is, of course, much firat is true in fills supposifion (Sfipek, 2001), and indeed, in fire United Stts this is a common belief. At fire same fime, at least some of fire difficulties children experience at school, eifirer wifir Ieaming or behavior, can eifirer begin in school or be exacerbated by experience firere. To date, firis reciprocal relafion is sfill insufficienfiy studied and understood (Sfipak, 2001). The understnding of what is school readiness is one firat is currenfiy under much discussion (Carlton 8 Winsler, 1999; Crnic 8 Lamberty, 1994; Holloway, Rambaud, Fuller, 8 Eggers-Piarola, 1995; May 8 Kundert, 1997). Alfirough firis study did not examine teacher atfitudes, ballot, or component of teacher training, fire role of fire teacher in 75 gr fostering successful and producfive classroom experience is well documented (Brookfiald, 1995; Rodgers-Jenkins 8 Chapman, 1990). Research has also heightened our awareness firat fire influence adult’ implicit and tcit beliefs about fire nature of fire child and fire purpose of curriculum are reflected in fire way content is presented and expectafions for children are defined (Brookfiald, 1995). Expectfions, for example, from fire Ministry of Educafion support a craafiva and well-managed early childhood program wifir many opportunifies for individual free-choice acfivifies, and small and large group acfivities. Expectfions, however, from parent stress academic achievement, rote learning, and less creafive endeavors as markers of a successful school experience, even for young children (Johnson 8 Brown, 1995; Morrison et al., 1998). Children may come to school wifir one set of expectfions from firelr parent and experience anofirer set of expectfions from firair teachers and not know how to negofiate fire territory. Parentl discipline styles may also influence how children are prepared to handle fire openness of a more construcfivist classroom environment (Lafltowit, Huesman, Eron, 1978; Parts, Cuents, 8 Zady, 2000; Parts, Sandu, Cuents, 8 Zady, 1995). Children who have been roared wifir physical punishment as fire primary means of discipline often have difficulfies in environment where fire emphasis is on choice and cooperafive learning (Brenner 8 Fox, 1998). If firis is fire case, and fire Ministry of Educafion want to promote more construcfivist forms of early 76 childhood educafion, teachers will have to be well-trained in fire forms of discipline and guidance firat facilitte firis type of Ieaming. To successfully ameliorate firis problem, ways to support bofir fire family and school will have to be considered, developed, and implemented. Result of future research can inform policies on mantl healfir in families and it ralafion to child outcome and suggesfions for needed support in light of fire many stressors found in Jamaican society. Future research can also inform issues of teacher training so firat teachers can successfully meet fire expectfions of fire Ministry of Educafion and meet fire needs of fire children firay are serving. 77 APPENDICES 78 -"."I}|~‘3Il APPENDIX A Freguency Tables for Variables in Anal ed and Archiva Models 79 Table 6 : quency Table of Puzzle Solving Scores from fire McCarfiry Scales of Child Devplopment PUZZLE SOLVING Frequency Percent Valid Cumulafive Percent Percent Valid 24 1 1 7.3 7.3 7.3 27 1 0 6.6 6.6 1 3.9 30 1 5 9.9 9.9 23.8 36 24 15.9 1 5.9 39.7 39 5 3.3 3.3 43.0 45 1 2 7.9 7.9 51 .0 48 7 4.6 4.6 55.6 51 9 6.0 6.0 61 .6 54 1 0 6.6 6.6 68.2 57 1 2 7.9 7.9 76.2 60 5 3.3 3.3 79.5 63 9 6.0 6.0 85.4 64 1 .7 .7 86.1 66 5 3.3 3.3 89.4 69 3 2.0 2.0 91 .4 75 3 2.0 2.0 93.4 81 3 2.0 2.0 95.4 84 1 .7 .7 96.0 90 1 .7 .7 96.7 93 1 .7 .7 97.4 96 1 .7 .7 98.0 1 02 3 2.0 2.0 100.0 Total 1 51 1 00.0 1 00.0 80 Table 7: Fgguency Table of Visual Factor Scores from fire McCarfiry Scales of Child Development VISUAL FACTOR Frequency Percent Valid Cumulafive Percent Percent Valid 50-59 3 2.1 2.1 2.0 60-69 13 8.9 8.9 10.6 70-79 16 10.7 10.7 21.2 80-89 28 18.5 18.5 39.7 90-99 34 22.5 22.5 92.3 100-109 30 19.9 19.9 82.1 110-119 19 12.6 12.6 94.7 ' 120-129 4 2.7 2.7 97.4 130-139 3 2.1 2.1 99.3 . 140-144 1 .7 .7 100 Totl 151 100 100 81 Table 8: Fmguency Table of Conceptual Grouping Scores from fire McCarfiry Scales of Child Development CONCEPTUAL GROUPING Frequency Percent Valid Cumulafive Percent Percent Valid 24 1 .7 .7 .7 27 1 3 8.6 8.6 9.3 33 1 5 9.9 9.9 1 9.2 36 1 2 7.9 7.9 27.2 42 9 6.0 6.0 33.1 45 14 9.3 9.3 42.4 51 1 1 7.3 7.3 49.7 57 22 14.6 14.6 64.2 66 24 1 5.9 1 5.9 80.1 75 1 1 7.3 7.3 87.4 96 1 1 7.3 7.3 94.7 1 02 8 5.3 5.3 1 00.0 Total 151 1 00.0 1 00.0 82 Table 9: Fggupncy Table of Verbal Factor Scores f_rpm fire McCarfiry Scales of Child Development Frequency Percent VERBAL FACTOR Valid 49-59 5 60-69 17 70-79 21 80-89 29 90-99 32 100-109 19 1 10-119 15 1 20-1 29 8 130-139 2 1 40-149 3 1 51 Totl 3.4 1 1 .3 1 3.8 1 9.3 21 .1 1 2.6 9.9 5.3 1.4 2.1 100 Valid Percent 3.4 1 1 .3 1 3.8 19.3 21 .1 12.6 9.9 5.3 1 .4 2.1 100 83 Cumulative Percent 3.3 14.6 28.5 47.7 68.9 81 .5 91 .4 96.7 98.0 100 Table 10: quency Table of Mofirers’ Educafional Attinment Mofirers educafion Frequency Percent Valid Percent Cumulafive Percent Valid did not completed 1 .7 .7 .7 primarylall-age completed primary 9 6.0 6.3 7.0 completed all-age 21 13.9 14.7 21.7 completed 105 69.5 73.4 95.1 secondarylhighltechnlcal completed secretarial! 5 3.3 3.5 98.6 commercial college completed university 2 1.3 1.4 100.0 Total 143 94.7 100.0 Missing 99 8 5.3 Total 151 100.0 84 prle 11: Fppguency Table of Parent’ Ralafionship Sttus Parant’ ielafionship Frequency Percent Valid Percent Cumulafive Percent Valid married 35 23.2 23.2 23.2 living 39 25.8 25.8 49.0 together visifing 13 8.6 8.6 57.6 separated 53 35.1 35.1 92.7 none 11 7.3 7.3 100.0 Totl 151 100.0 100.0 85 Tabjp 12: quency Distribufion of Mofirer’s Age Mofirer’s Age Frequency Percent Valid Cumulafive Percent Percent Valid 19 1 .7 .7 .7 20 2 1 .3 1 .3 2.0 21 4 2.6 2.6 4.6 22 4 2.6 2.6 7.3 23 10 6.6 6.6 13.9 24 4 2.6 2.6 16.6 25 3 2.0 2.0 18.5 26 18 11.9 11.9 30.5 27 11 7.3 7.3 37.7 28 6 4.0 4.0 41 .7 29 6 4.0 4.0 45.7 30 9 6.0 6.0 51 .7 31 7 4.6 4.6 56.3 32 13 8.6 8.6 64.9 33 7 4.6 4.6 69.5 34 4 2.6 2.6 72.2 35 7 4.6 4.6 78.8 36 2 1 .3 1 .3 78.1 37 3 2.0 2.0 80.1 38 3 2.0 2.0 82.1 39 9 6.0 6.0 88.1 40 6 4.0 4.0 92.1 41 3 2.0 2.0 94.0 42 4 2.6 ‘ 2.6 96.7 43 2 1 .3 1 .3 98.0 45 ‘l .7 .7 98.7 46 1 .7 .7 99.3 51 1 .7 .7 100.0 Tot 151 100.0 100.0 86 Table 13: quency Tgble of Child IQ §pores CHILD‘S IQ. Frequency Percent Valid Cumulafive Percent Percent Valid 60-69 15 10.0 10.0 9.9 70-79 23 15.2 15.2 25.2 80-89 34 22.5 22.5 47.7 90-99 41 27.0 27.0 74.8 100-109 24 16.0 16.0 90.7 110-119 10 6.5 6.5 97.4 120-129 2 1.3 1.3 98.7 130-139 2 1.4 1.4 100.0 151 100 100 87 Table 14: Fgguency Table of Study Chilgren in Six Basic Schools Schools Frequency Percent Valid Cumulative Percent Percent Valid Water Corn 30 19.9 19.9 19.9 Providence 31 20.5 20.5 40.4 August Twn 1 5 9.9 9.9 50.3 Mona Com 13 8.6 8.6 58.9 Hope Val 31 20.5 20.5 79.5 Shady Gr 31 20.5 20.5 100.0 Totl 151 100.0 100.0 88 Table 15: Fggupncy Iable of Agp of Study Children Child’s Age Valid Totl 3.0 — 3.99 4.0 - 4.99 Frequency Percent Valid 26 41 52 32 151 Percent 17.2 17.2 27.2 27.2 34.4 34.4 21.2 21.2 100 100 89 Cum ulafive Percent 1 7.2 44.4 78.8 1 00.0 Table 16: quency Tabla pf Totl Socioeconomic Sttus of Families of Study Children totlses Frequency Percent Valid Cumulafive Percent Percent Valid 6.00 1 .7 .7 .7 9.00 2 1.3 1.3 2.0 10.00 3 2.0 2.0 4.0 11.00 4 2.6 2.6 6.6 12.00 8 5.3 5.3 11.9 13.00 8 5.3 5.3 17.2 14.00 10 6.6 6.6 23.8 15.00 15 9.9 9.9 33.8 16.00 24 15.9 15.9 49.7 17.00 25 16.6 16.6 66.2 18.00 37 24.5 24.5 90.7 19.00 14 9.3 9.3 f 100.0 Totl 151 100.0 100.0 90 Table 17: Fppguency Table of Immatur_l_ty' Scores from fire Connors Behavioral Scales Immatrlty Frequency Percent Valid Cumulafive Percent Percent Valid .00 4 2.6 2.6 2.6 1.00 7 4.6 4.6 7.3 2.00 6 4.0 4.0 11.3 3.00 12 7.9 7.9 19.2 4.00 8 5.3 5.3 24.5 5.00 14 9.3 9.3 33.8 6.00 12 7.9 7.9 41.7 7.00 9 6.0 6.0 47.7 8.00 13 8.6 8.6 56.3 9.00 13 8.6 8.6 64.9 10.00 7 4.6 4.6 69.5 11.00 9 6.0 6.0 75.5 12.00 2 1.3 1.3 76.8 13.00 5 3.3 3.3 80.1 14.00 8 5.3 5.3 85.4 15.00 2 1.3 1.3 86.8 16.00 5 3.3 3.3 90.1 17.00 4 2.6 2.6 92.7 18.00 1 .7 .7 93.4 19.00 2 1.3 1.3 94.7 20.00 2 1.3 1.3 96.0 22.00 1 .7 .7 96.7 23.00 1 .7 .7 97.4 25.00 2 1.3 1.3 98.7 26.00 1 .7 .7 99.3 34.00 1 .7 .7 100.0 Total 151 100.0 100.0 Table 18: quency Table of Hymracfivigy Scale from fire Connors Behavioral Scale Hyperacfivity Frequency Percent Valid Cumulafive Percent Percent Valid 1 .00 2 1 .3 1 .3 1 .3 3.00 2 1.3 1 .3 2.6 4.00 3 2.0 2.0 4.6 5.00 4 2.6 2.6 7.3 6.00 1 .7 .7 7.9 7.00 9 6.0 6.0 1 3.9 8.00 4 2.6 2.6 1 6.6 9.00 14 9.3 9.3 25.8 1 0.00 6 4.0 4.0 29.8 1 1 .00 1 1 7.3 7.3 37.1 1 2.00 1 3 8.6 8.6 45.7 1 3.00 8 5.3 5.3 51 .0 1 4.00 1 1 7.3 7.3 58.3 1 5.00 5 3.3 3.3 61.6 1 6.00 1 1 7.3 7.3 68.9 1 7.00 5 3.3 3.3 72.2 1 8.00 1 0 6.6 6.6 78.8 1 9.00 2 1.3 1 .3 80.1 20.00 6 4.0 4.0 84.1 21 .00 3 2.0 2.0 86.1 22.00 4 2.6 2.6 88.7 23.00 5 3.3 3.3 92.1 24.00 4 2.6 2.6 94.7 25.00 3 2.0 2.0 96.7 26.00 3 2.0 2.0 98.7 27.00 1 .7 .7 99.3 29.00 1 .7 .7 1 00.0 Total 1 51 1 00.0 1 00.0 92 I11 I—-I Table 19: quency Table of Family Cohesion Factor on fire FACES Family Cohesion Score Frequency Percent Valid Cumulafive Percent Percent Valid 1 0.00 1 .7 .7 .7 1 1 .00 2 1 .3 1 .3 2.0 1 2.00 1 .7 .7 2.6 1 3.00 2 1 .3 1 .3 4.0 14.00 2 1 .3 1 .3 5.3 1 6.00 8 5.3 5.3 1 0.6 1 7.00 3 2.0 2.0 1 2.6 1 9.00 3 2.0 2.0 14.6 20.00 6 4.0 4.0 1 8.5 21 .00 6 4.0 4.0 22.5 22.00 4 2.6 2.6 25.2 23.00 5 3.3 3.3 28.5 24.00 6 4.0 4.0 32.5 25.00 8 5.3 5.3 37.7 26.00 1 6 1 0.6 1 0.6 48.3 27.00 1 0 6.6 6.6 55.0 28.00 1 3 8.6 8.6 63.6 29.00 1 3 8.6 8.6 72.2 30.00 1 0 6.6 6.6 78.8 31 .00 8 5.3 5.3 84.1 32.00 7 4.6 4.6 88.7 33.00 5 3.3 3.3 92.1 34.00 8 5.3 5.3 97.4 35.00 2 1 .3 1 .3 98.7 36.00 2 1 .3 1 .3 1 00.0 Total 1 51 100.0 100.0 93 Table 20: Fgguency Table of fire Qfifirymia Scores on fire Brief Smptom Inventol_'y Dysfirymia Frequency Percent Valid Cumulafive Percent Percent Valid .00 39 25.8 25.8 25.8 1.00 23 15.2 15.2 41.1 2.00 10 6.6 6.6 47.7 3.00 15 9.9 9.9 57.6 4.00 9 6.0 6.0 63.6 5.00 8 5.3 5.3 68.9 6.00 13 8.6 8.6 77.5 7.00 7 4.6 4.6 82.1 8.00 5 3.3 3.3 85.4 9.00 1 .7 .7 86.1 10.00 2 1.3 1.3 87.4 1 1 .00 4 2.6 2.6 90.1 12.00 4 2.6 2.6 92.7 13.00 3 2.0 2.0 94.7 14.00 4 2.6 2.6 97.4 15.00 1 .7 .7 98.0 16.00 1 .7 .7 98.7 17.00 2 1.3 1.3 100.0 Totl 151 100.0 100.0 94 Table 21: Freguency Table of Psychofic Scores on fire Brief Symptom Inventoi_'y Psychofic Valid .00 1.00 2.00 3.00 4.00 5.00 6.00 7.00 8.00 9.00 10.00 1 1 .00 12.00 15.00 18.00 19.00 22.00 Totl Frequency Percent Valid Percent 63 20 21 1 3 fiN-i-l-l-INNN-lmm-h‘l .3 GI 41 .7 1 3.2 13.9 8.6 4.6 2.6 3.3 Jul-h .u OUN‘INNUUGNU .s 9 ca 95 41 .7 1 3.2 1 3.9 8.6 4.6 2.6 3.3 _ ut-b-l u OUNNNNQQQN“ d O Q-l Cumulafive Percent 41 .7 55.0 68.9 77.5 82.1 84.8 88.1 91 .4 92.1 93.4 94.7 . 96.0 96.7 97.4 98.0 98.7 100.0 _ ' 1'? '.;'§' .- :-.'_r‘-i' APPENDIX B Freguency Distribufions for Variables in Analyzed and Archival Models 96 Mother's Age 3‘ 5 Std. Dev = 6.44 § Mean = 30.9 LII: N = 151.00 20.0 25.0 30.0 35.0 40.0 45.0 50.0 22.5 27.5 32.5 37.5 42.5 47.5 Mother's Age Figure 6: Frequency Distribufion of Mofirers’ Age 97 parents relationship 60 ‘ Std. Dev = 1.34 Mean = 2.8 N = 151.00 Frequency 1.0 2.0 3.0 4.0 5.0 parents relationship Figure 7: Frequency Distribufion of Parent’ Ralafionship 98 Mothers education 120 g Std. Dev = .71 §. Mean = 3.8 L: N = 143.00 1.0 2.0 3.0 4.0 5.0 8.0 Mothers education Figure 8: Frequency Distribufion of Mofireis’ Educafion 99 he VERBAL FACTOR 30 6‘ 5 Std. Dev= 19.01 3 Mean 2 91.2 g N =151.00 $30 ob-o )o-o ob-o $0 ’97 ”'00 (320 {120 ,0 {$20 VERBAL FACTOR Figure 9: Frequency Disfilbufion of Verbal Factor Scores from fire McCarfiry Scales of Child Development 100 VISUAL FACTOR 6‘ 5 Std. Dev = 17.25 §' Mean = 93.2 I: N = 151.00 0 ) ) .9 I I 7 I I I I I I %o‘ttoeo'oqoo-oqoob-oqigbo‘i‘ooo 06‘ ’0 ’3“? ‘30‘ ‘9 ‘9‘ 700%.} VISUAL FACTOR Figure 10: Frequency Distribufion of Visual Factor Scores from fire McCarfiry Scales of Child Development 101 CONCEPTUAL GROUPING 30 5‘ 5 Std. Dev = 21.50 g. Mean = 55.7 1: o N = 151.00 20.0 30.0 40.0 50.0 60.0 70.0 80.0 90.0 100.0 CONCEPTUAL GROUPING Figure 11: Frequency Distribufion of Conceptual Grouping Scores from fire McCarfiry Scales of Child Development 102 school 40 Frequency School 1 School 2 School 3 School 4 School 5 School 6 Std. Dev=1.88 Mean=3.5 N=151.00 Water Commission Providence August Township Mona Commission Hope Valley Shady Grove Figure 12: Frequency Distribufion of Study Children at Basic Schools 103 test1 age Frequency 777663190 %%%%tsttsssts test1 age Figure 13: Frequency Distribufion of Age of Sfildy Children totalses 70 60: 50: 401 301 Std. Dev = 2.53 Mean = 15.9 N = 151.00 6.0 8.0 10.0 12.0 14.0 16.0 18.0 20.0 totlsos Figure 14: Frequency Distribufion of Totl Socioeconomic Status of Families of Study Children 105 Immaturity Std. Dev = 5.94 Mean = 8.7 N =151.00 0.0 5.0 10.0 15.0 20.0 25.0 30.0 35.0 2.5 7.5 12.5 17.5 22.5 27.5 32.5 Immaturity Figure 15: Frequency Distribufion of Immaturity Scores from fire Connors Behavioral Scales 106 Hyperactivity Std. Dev = 5.99 Mean = 14.0 N = 151.00 0.0 5.0 10.0 15.0 20.0 25.0 30.0 2.5 7.5 12.5 17.5 22.5 27.5 HYDGTHCIIVIIY Figure 16: Frequency Distribufion of Hyperacfivity Factor Scores from fire Connors Behavioral Scale 107 Family Cohesion 40 Frequency Family Cohesion Figure 17: Frequency Distribufion of Family Cohesion Factor Scores on fire FACES 108 Dysthymia 50 4O 30 20 10 g Std. Dev=4.38 é Mean =4.1 I: o N =151.00 0.0 2.0 4.0 6.0 8.0 10.0 12.014.016.0 18.0 DYSthymia Figure 18: Frequency Distribufion of Dysthymia Factor Scores on fire Brief Symptom Inventory 109 Psychotc 100 5‘ 5 Std. Dev=4.12 a?” Mean =2.6 u: N =151.00 Psychotc Figure 19: Frequency Distribufion of Psychofic Factor Scores on fire Brief Symptom Inventory 110 APPENDIX C ARCHIVAL MODELS 111 Family Cohesion Verbal l l Total sss l [ Immaturity T— -.24** Psychofic *=p<95 **=p<.01 x“ = 2.963 DF=3 P = 0.397 Figure 20: Archival Modal: Influence of SES, Maternal Psychofic Behavior, and Family Cohesion on Child Outcome 112 [ Hyporacfivity 1*; ' .41 Family Cohesion -.24 Totl SES \ -.35* —Pl Dysfirymia * =p<.05 ** =p<.01 x’ = 7.116 DF=6 P = 3.310 Child’s IO 1 l Immaturity -.24** l Hyperacfivity I* .39 Figure 21: Archival Model: Influence of SES, Matrnal Dysfirymia, and Family Cohesion on Child Outcome 113 REFERENCES Adams, J. (1989). A descripfion of some parcepfions in Jamaican immigrant. In V. Clarke (Ed.), Estblishing now lives: Selected readings in Caribbean immigrant in New York. New York: Caribbean Research Center, Madgar Evers College, City University of New York. Amato, P. (2000). Diversity wifirin single-parent families. In D. H. Demo 8 K. R. Allen (Eds.), Handbook of family diversity (pp. 149-172). New York: Oxford University Press. Arbuckle, J. L., 8 Wofirke, W. (1999). Amos usprs guide. Chicago, IL: Smallwaters Corporafion. American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Mood Disorders, Diagnosfic and stfisfical manual of mantl disordprs (4fir ed., pp. 213-233). Washington, 0.0.: American Psychiatric Associafion. Baker, B. L., 8 Heller, T. L. (1996). Preschool children wifir externalizing behaviors: Experience of fafirers and mofirers. Journal of Abnormal Child PsycholggyI 24(4), 513-532. Barfilow, H. A. (1997). The debt dilemma: IMF nagofiafions in Jamaicall anadaI and prana. London: Macmillan. Baumrind, D. (1971). Harmonious parent and firelr preschool children. Developmentl Psycholpgy, 4(1, Pt. 1), 99-102. Beitchman, J. H., Wilson, 8., Brownlie, E. B., 8 Walters, H. (1996). Long- terrn consistency in speechllanguage profiles: ll. Behavioral, emofional, and social outcomes. Journal of fire American Academy of Child 8: Adolescent PsychiaflI 35(6), 815-825. Benasich, A. A., 8 Curfiss, S. (1993). Language, learning, and behavioral disturbances in childhood: A longitudinal perspecfive. Journal of American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiat_ryI 32(3), 585- 594. Biafora, F. (1995). Cross-cultural perspacfive and wellness: lmplicafions for depression. Journal of Social Distress 8 me Homeless, 4(2), 105-129. Billings, A. G., 8 Moos, R. H. (1983). Comparison of children of depressed and nondepressed parent: A social-environmentl perspecfiva. Jggrnal of Abnormal Child Psycholpgy, 1 1(4), 463-485. 114 Billings, A. G., 8 Moos, R. H. (1986). Children of parent wifir unipolar depression: A controlled 1-year follow-up. Journal of Abnormal Child Psycholggy, 14(1), 149-166. Brenner, V., 8 Fox, R. A. (1998). Parentl discipline and behavior problems in young children. Journal of Genefic Psycholgy, 159(2), 251 -256. Brice-Baker, J. (1996). Jamaican families. In M. McGoldrick 8 J. Gordono 8 J. K. Pearce (Eds.), Efirnicjty and Family Therapy (pp. 84-96). New York: Guilford. Brigman, G., Lana, D., Switer, D., Lane, D., 8 Lawrence, R. (1999). Teaching children school success skills. Journal of Educafional Research, 92(6), 323-329. Bronfenbrennor, U. (1986). Ecology of fire family as a context for human development: Research perspecfives. Development and Psycholpgy, 23(6), 723-742. Brookfield, S. D. (1995). Becoming a crifically reflective teacher. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. 296 pp. Brooks-Gunn, J., Duncan, G., 8 Britto, P. R. (1999). Are socioeconomic gradient for children similar to firose for adult?: Achievement and healfir of children in fire United Sttas. In D. Keafing 8 C. Hartman (Eds.), Developmentl healfir and waalfir of nafipns: Social, biological, and educafional dynamics (pp. 94-124). New York: The Guildford Press. Brooks-Gunn, J., Klebanov, P. K., Llaw, F., 8 Duncan, G. (1995). Toward an understnding of fire effect of poverty upon children. Brown, J., Newland, A., Anderson, P., 8 Chevannes, B. (1997). Caribbean fafirerhood: Underrasoarched, misunderstood. In J. L. Roopnarina 8 J. Brown (Eds.), Caribbean families: Diversity among afirnic groupp (pp. 85-114). Greenwich, CT: Ablex Publishing Corporafion. Bukowskl, W. M., 8 Sippola, L. K. (1998). Diversity and fire social mind: Goals, construct, culture, and development. Developmentl Peycholggy, 34(4), 742-746. Bullock, J. R., 8 Pennington, D. (1988). The relafionship between parentl percepfions of fire family environment and children's perceived competence. Child Study Journal, 18(1), 17-31. 115 Campbell, S. B. (1987). Parent-referred problem firree-year-olds: Developmentl changes in symptoms. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiat_ry and Allied Disciplines, 28(6), 835-834. Campbell, S. B. (1990). Behavior problems in preschool children: Clinical and davelopmentl issues. New York: The Guilford Press. Campbell, 8. B. (1991). Longitudinal studies of acfiva and aggressive preschoolers: Individual differences in early behavior and outcomes. In D. Cicchetfi 8 S. L. Tofir (Eds.), Intemalizing and externalizing expressions of dypfuncfion (pp. 57-89). Hillsdale NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. Campbell, S. B. (1995). Behavior problems in preschool children: A review of recent research. Journal of Child Psycholpgy and Psychlagy 8 Allied Disciplines, 36(1), 113-149. Campbell, S. B. (1998). Developmentl perspecfives. In T. H. Ollendick 8 M. Herson (Eds.), Handbook of child gychopafirolpgy (3rd ed. ed., pp. 3-35). New York: Plenum Press. Campbell, S. B., Cohn, J. F., 8 Meyers, T. (1995). Depression in first-fima mofirers: Mofirer-infant interacfion and depression chronicity. Developmentl Psycholpgy, 31(3), 349-357. Campbell, S. B., 8 Ewing, L. J. (1990). Follow-up of hard-to-manage preschoolers: Adjustment at age 9 and predictors of confinuing symptoms. Journal of Child Psycholpgy and Psychiat_ry 8 Allied Dipciplinas, 31(6), 871 -889. Campbell, S. 8., March, C., Pierce, E. W., 8 Ewing, L. J. (1991a). Hard to manage preschool boys: Family context and fire stbility of externalizing behavior. Journal of Abnormal Child Psycholpgy, 19(3), 301 -318. Campbell, S. B., 8 Pierce, E. W. (1996). Boys' extemallzing problems at elementry school age: Pathways from early behavior problems, maternal control, and family stress. Development and Peychopgtholpgy, 8, 701 -719. Campbell, S. 3., Pierce, E. W., March, C., 8 Ewing, L. J. (1991b). Noncompliant behavior, overacfivity, and family stress as predictors of negafive maternal control wifir preschool children. Development and Psychopafirolpgy, 3(2), 175-190. 116 Campbell, S. 8., Pierce, E. W., March, C., 8 Ewing, L. J. (1994). Hard-to- manage preschool boys: Symptomafic behavior across context and time. Child Development, 65(3), 836-851. Carlton, M. P., 8 Winsler, A. (1999). School readiness: The need for a paradigm shift. School Psycholpgy Review, 28(3), 338-352. Carson, D. K., Klee, T., Perry, C. K., Muskina, G., 8 Donagy, T. (1998). Comparisons of children wifil delayed and normal language at 24 monfirs of age on measures of behavioral difficulfies, social, and cognifive development. Internal Mentl Healfir Journal, 19(1), 59-75. Case, R., Griffin, S., 8 Kelly, W. (1999). Socioeconomic gradient in mathemafical ability and firoir responsiveness to intrvenfion during early childhood. In D. Keafing 8 C. Hartman (Eds.), Developmentl health and fire wealfir of nafions: Social, biological, and educafional dynamics (pp. 125-149). New York: The Guilford Press. Case, R., Griffin, S., 8 Kelly, W. (2001). Socioeconomic differences in children's early cognifive development and fireir readiness for schooling. In S. Golbeck (Ed.), Psycholpgical pprsmfives on early childhood educafion: Reframing dilemmas in research and pracfice (pp. 37-63). Mahwah NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. Caspi, A. Moffitt, T., Newman, D., 8 Silva P.A. (1997). Behavioral observafions at age 3 years predict adult psychiatric disorders: Longitudinal evidence from a birth cohort. In M. E. Hertig 8 E. A. Farber (Eds.), Annual prpgress in child gychiagy and child development (pp. 319-331). Clayton, P. J. (1998). Depression subtyping: Treatment implicafions. Journpl pf Clinical Psychiagy, 59(16), 5-12. Cole, D. A., 8 Jordan, A. E. (1989). Assessment of cohesion and adaptbility in component family dyads: A quosfion of convergent and discriminant validity. Journal of Counseling Psycholgy, 36(4), 456-463. Cole, M. (1999). Culture in development. In M. H. Bornstein 8 M. Lamb (Eds.), Developmentl ppycholpgy: An advanced textbook (4fir ed., pp. 73-123). 117 Committee for Early Childhood Pedagogy, Com mission on Behavioral and Social Sciences and Educafion, Nafional Research Council, (2001). Barbara Bowman, M. Suzanne Donovan, 8 M. Susan Burns (Eds.) Eager to Learn: Educafing our preschoolers. Washington: Nafional Academy Press. Coolahan, K., Fantuzzo, J., Mendez, J., 8 McDermott, P. (2000). Preschool peer Interacfions and readiness to loam: Relafionships between classroom peer play and learning behaviors and conduct. Journal of Educafional Epychology, 92(3), 458-465. Crick, N. R., 8 Dodge, K. A. (1994). A review and reformulafion of social infonnafion-processing mechansims in children's social adjustment. Psycholggical Bulletin, 1 1 5(1), 74-1 01 . Crick, N. R., 8 Dodge, K. A. (1996). Social lnforrnafion-processing mechanisms on reacfive and proacfive aggression. Chilg ngelopment, 67(3), 993-1002. Crnic, K., 8 Lamberty, G. (1994 Apr). Reconsidering school readiness: Conceptual and applied perspecfives. Eaply Education and Development, 5(2), 91 -105. Cummings, E. M., Iannotfi, R. J., 8 Zahn-Waxler, C. (1989). Aggression between peers in early childhood: Individual confinuity and developmentl change. Child vaelopmept, 60, 887-895. Darling, N., 8 Steinberg, L. (1993). Parenfing style as context: An integrafiva model. Psycholpgical Bullefin, 113(3), 487-496. Davies, D. (1999). Child develoment: A pracfioner's guide. New York: Guilford. DeKIyan, M., Biambaum, M. A., Greenberg, M. T., 8 Spelt, M. L. (1998). Fafirers and preschool behavior problems. vaplopmentl Ps oh I 34(2), 264-275. Darogafis, L. R. (1992). Brief Symptom Inventogy (BSI): Adminisfiafion, scoring, and procedures manual. Third Edifion. Derogatis, L. R. (1993). Brief Symmm Inventol_'y. Minneapolis, MN: Nafional Computer Systems. Dix, T. (1991). The Affecfive Organizafion of Parenfing: Adapfive and Maladapfive Processes. Epycholpgical Bullefin, 110(1), 3-25. 118 Dodge, K. A. (1990). Developmentl Psychopafirology in Children of Depressed Mofirers. Developmentl Psychology, 26(1), 3-6. Dohrenwend, B. P. (1990). Socioeconomic sttus (SES) and psychiatric disorders: Are the issues sfill compelling? Social Psychiat_ry and Psychiatric Epidemiolpgy, 25(1), 41 -47. Dohrenwend, B. P., Levav, I., Shrout, P. E., 8 Schwart, S. (1992). Socioeconomic sttus and psychiatric disorders: The casuafion- selocfion issue. Science 255(5047), 946-952. Doucette-Gates, A., Brooks-Gunn, J., 8 Chase-Lansdale, P. L. (1998). The role of bias and equivalence in fire study of race, class, and efirnicity. In V. C. McLoyd 8 L. Steinberg (Eds.), Studying minority adolescent: Conceptual, methodological, and firoorefical issug (pp. 211-236). Mahwah NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associats. Downey, G., 8 Coyne, J. C. (1990). Children of depressed parent: An integrafivo review. Psycholpgical Bullefin, 108(1), 50-76. Duncan, G., Brooks-Gunn, J., 8 Klebanov, P. K. (1994). Economic doprivafion and early childhood development. Child Development Sgcial Issue, 65(2), 296-318. Duncan, G., 8 Young, W. J. (1998). How much does child poverty affect fire life chances of children? American Sociolpgical Review, 63(June), 406-423. Durbrow, E. H. (1999). Cultural processes in child competence: How rural caribboan pparont evaluate fireir children. In A. Masten (Ed.), Cultural Processes in Child Development (Vol. 29, pp. 92-122). Mahwah NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Eisert, D. C., Walker, H. M., Severson, H., 8 Block, A. (1988). Patterns of social-behavioral competence in behavior disordered preschoolers. Early Child Development 8 Care, 41, 139-152. Ekman, P., 8 Davidson, R. J. (Eds.). (1994). The nature of emofions: Fundamentl guosfions. New York: Oxford University Press. Evans, H. L. (1989). Parsmfives on fire socializafion of fire working-class Jamaican child (Vol. 38). Field, T. M. (1995). Psychologically depressed parent. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of paronfing (Vol. 4: Applied and pracfical parenfing, pp. 85-99). Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associats. Inc. 119 Floyd, F. J., 8 Widaman, K. F. (1995). Factor analysis in fire development and refinement of clinical assessment instrument. Psychological Assossment:Sgcial Issue: Mefirodological issues in gycholpgical assessment research, 7(3), 286-299. Geert, C. (1973). The inte_r_prettion of cultures. New York: Basic Books. Goldsmifir, D. F., 8 Rogoff, B. (1995). Sensifivlty and teaching by dysphoric and nondysphoric women in structured versus unstructured situafions. Developmentl Psycholgy, 31(3), 388-394. Goldsmifir, D. F., 8 Rogoff, B. (1997). Mofirers' and toddlers' coordinated joint focus of attenfion: Variafions wifir maternal dysphoric symptoms. Developmentl Psycholpgy, 33(1), 113-119. Gonzalez-Ramos, G., Zayas, L. H., 8 Cohen, E. V. (1998). Child-roaring values of low-income, urban Puerto Rican mofirers of preschool children. Professional Psycholpgy: Research 8 Pracfico, 29(4), 377- 382. Goodman, S. H., 8 Gofiib, l. H. (1999). Risk for psychopafirology in fire children of depressed mofirors: A developmentl model for understnding mechanisms of transmission. Psycholpgical Review, 196(3). 458-490. GopauI-McNicol, S. (1998). Cariboan families: Social and emofional problems. Journal of Social Distress 8 fire Homeless, 7(1), 55-73. Gopaul-McNicol, S. (1999). Efilnocultural porspocfivos on childrearing pracfices in fire Caribbean. Internafional Social Work 42(1), 79-86. Gorman-Smifir, D., Tolan, P. H., Zelli, A., 8 Huesmann, L. R. (1996). The relafion of family funcfioning to violence among inner-city youfirs. Journal of Family Psycholpgy, 10 (2), 115-129. Gottrnan, M. J., Kat, F., 8 Hooven, P. (1997). Met-omofion: How families communicate emofionally. Mahwah NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Goyette, C. H., Connors, C. K., 8 Ulrich, R. F. (1978). Norrnafivo dat on revised Connors parent and teacher rafing scales. Journal of Abnormal Child Psycholpgy, 6(2), 221 -236. 120 Gralinskl, J., 8 Kopp, C. B. (1993). Everyday rules for behavior: Mofirers' request to young children. Developmentl Psycholpgy, 29(3), 573- 584. Grant, D. R. B. (1974). Living condifions of some Basic School children: Pointers to disadvantge. Kingston, Jamaica: Bernard Van Leer Foundafion for Early Childhood Educafion. Grant, D. R. B. (1984). Early Childhood Educafion: Funcfional lnteracfivity of Family—Community-School (Posifion paper; Researchltochnical report Eric Number: ED244721). Kingston, Jamaica: Bernard Van Leer Foundafion for Early Childhood Educafion. Greenberg, M. T., Lengua, L. J., Cole, J. D., 8 Pinderhughes, E. E. (1999). Predicfing developmentl outcomes at school entry using a . muIfipIe-risk model: Four American communlfies. Developmentl Peycholmy, 35(2), 403-417. Hasfings, P. D., 8 Rubin, K. H. (1999). Predicfing mofirers' beliefs about preschool-aged children's social behavior: Evidence for maternal atfitudos moderafing child effect. Child Dovelppmont, 70(3), 722- 741. Haller, T. L., 8 Baker, B. L. (2000). Maternal negafivlty in children's externalizing behavior. Early Educafion and Development, 11(4), 483- 498. Hohn, G. (1996). The effect of family funcfioning on fire psychological and social adjustment of Jamaican immigrant children. Dissortfion Abstract Internafional: Socfion B: The Sciences and Engineering, gas-B), 341 1 . Hollingshead, A. B. (1975). Four factor index of social sttus. New Haven, CT: Yale University, Department of Sociology. Holloway, S. D., Rambaud, M. F., Fuller, 8., 8 Eggers-Piorola, C. (1995). What is ”appropriate pracfico" at home and in child care? Low- income mofirors' views on preparing fireir children for school. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 10(4), 451 -473. Hops, H. (1995). Ago- and gender-specific effect of parentl depression: A commentry. Developmentl Psycholpgy, 31(3), 428-431. Hu, L.-T., 8 Benfior, P. M. (1995). Evaluafing model fit. In R. H. Hoyle (Ed.), Structural oguafion modeling: Concept, issues, and applicafions (pp. 76-99). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publicafions, Inc. 121 James, S. (1977). Obsorvafions of classroom space: lmplicafions for learning and teaching children in fire caribboan. Caribbean Journal of Educafion 4(3), 112-145. Johnson, M. A., 8 Brown, M. M. (1995). Croafivity and fire learning environment: Jamaican early childhood schools. Caribbean Journal of Educafion 17(2), 279-297. Johnson, V. K., Cowan, P. A., 8 Cowan, C. P. (1999). Children's classroom behavior: The unique contribufion of family organizafion. Journal of Family Psycholpgy, 13(3), 355-371. Kalpidou, M. D., Rofirbaum, F., 8 Rosen, K. S. (1998). A longitudinal study of mofirors' and preschool children's aversive behaviors during dyadic interacfions. The Journal of Genofic Psychology, 159(1), 103- 1 1 6. Kaufman, A. S., 8 Kaufman, N. L. (1977). Clinical Evaluafion of Young Children wifir fire McCarfiry Scales. New York: Grune 8 Stratton. Koafing, D., 8 Hartman, C. (Eds.). (1999). Developmentl healfir and fire wealfir of nafions: Social, biolpgical, and oducafional dynamics. New York: The Guilford Press. Keenan, K., 8 Shaw, D. (1994). The development of aggression in toddlers: A study of low-income families. Journal of Abnormal Child Psycholpgy, 22(1), 53-77. Klein, L. (1995). Pafir analysis. In L. G. Grimm 8 P. R. Yarnold (Eds.), Reading and understnding mulfivariate stfisfics (pp. 65-98). Washington, D. C.: American Psychological Associafion. Kupersmidt, J. B., Griesler, P. C., DeRosier, M. E., 8 Patterson, C. J. (1995). Childhood aggression and peer relafions in fire context of family and neighborhood factors. Child Development, 66(2), 360-375. Lambert, M., Knight, F., Taylor, 8 Achenbach, T. (1996). Comparisons of behavioral and emofional problems among children in Jamaica and fire United SttszTeacher report for ages 6-11. Journal of Cross- Cultural Psychology, 27(1), 82-97. Lambert, M., Weisz, J., 8 Knight, F. (1989a). Over and undercontrollod problems of Jamaican and American children and adolescent. Journal of Consulfing and Clinical Psycholpgy, 57, 467-472. 122 Lambert, M. C., Knight, F., Taylor, R., 8 Achenbach, T. (1994). Epidemiology of behavioral and omofional problems among children of Jamaica and fire United Sttos: Parent report for ages 6-11. Journal of Abnormal Child Psycholpgy, 22(1), 113-128. Lambert, M. C., Knight, F., Taylor, R., 8 Newell, A. L. (1993). Furfirer comparisons of teacher and parent rafings of behavior and emofional problems of Jamaican children. lnternafional Journal of Intercultural Rolafions 17(1), 1-18. Lambert, M. C., 8 Lyubansky, M. (1999). Behavior and omofional problems among Jamaican children and adolescent: An epidemiological survey of parent, teacher, and self-report for ages 6-18. lnternafional Journal of Intercultural Rolafions 23(5), 727-751. Lambert, M. C., Lyubansky, M., 8 Achenbach, T. (1998). Behavioral and omofional problems among adolescent of Jamaica and fire United Sttos: Parent, teacher, and self-report for ages 12 to 18. Journal of Emofional 8 Behavioral Disorders 6(3), 180-187. Lambert, M. C., Puig, M., Lyubansky, M., 8 Rowan, G. T. (2001a). Adult perspecfives on behavior and omofional problems in African American children. Journal of Black Psycholpgy, 27(1), 64-85. Lambert, M. C., Samms-Vaughan, M. E., Lyubansky, M., Podolski, C., Hannah, S. D., McCaslin, S. E., 8 Rowan, G. T. (1999). Behavior and emofional problems of clinic-referred children of fire African diaspora: A cross-nafional study of African American and Jamaican children ages 4 to 18. Journal of Black Psycholgy, 25(4), 504-523. Lambert, M. C., Schmltt, N., Samms-Vaughan, M. E., Russ, C. M., Lewis, K. M., Lancaster, K. E., 8 Orellana, C. (2001b). The family cohesion and adaptbility scale-ll (FACES-II: Is fire factor structure for Jamaicans similar to firat astblished in fire United Sttes. under review. Lambert, M. C., Thesiger, C., Overly, K., 8 Knight, F. (1990). Teacher and parent rafings of behavior problems in Jamaican children and adolescent: Convergence and divergence of views. lnternafional Journal of Intercultural Relafions 14(2), 177-191. Lambert, M. C., Weisz, J. R., 8 Thesiger, C. (1989b). Principal component analyses of behavior problems in Jamaican clinic-referred children: Teacher report for ages 6-17. Journal of Abnormal Child Psycholpgy, 17(5), 553-562. 123 Lang, C., Field, T. M., Pickens, J., 8 Marfinez, A. (1996). Preschoolers of dysphoric mofirers. Journal of Child Psycholpgy 8 Psychiagy 8 Allied Disciplines, 37(2), 221-224. Lave, J., 8 Wenger, E. (1991). Situated Ieaming: I_.pgifimate gripheral parficigfion. New York: Cambridge University Press. Leflrowlt, M. M., Huesmann, L. R., 8 Eron, L. D. (1978). Parentl punishment: A longitudinal analysis of effect. Archives of General Psychiat_ry, 35(2), 186-191. Leo-Rhynie, E. (1993). The Jamaican famljy: confinuity and change. Kingston: The Grace, Kennedy Foundafion. Leo-Rhynio, E., 8 Hamilton, M. (1993). Life-style as a measure of socio- economic sttus: The development of an instrument for use in Jamaica. Social and Economic Studies 35-52. Lindahl, K. M. (1998). Family process variables and children's disruptive behavior problems. Journal of Famlu Psycholpgy, 12(3), 420-436. Loeber, R. (1991). Anfi-social behavior: More enduring firan Changeable? Journal of American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psyphiagy, 32(3), 393-397. Loeber, R., 8 Dishion, T. (1983). Early predictors of male delinquency: A review. Psycholgoical Bullofin, 94(1), 68-99. Loeber, R., 8 Stoufiramor-Loebor, M. (1998). Development of juvenile aggression and violence: Some common misconcepfions and controversies. American Psycholpgist, 53(2), 242-259. Maccoby, E. E. (1992). The role of parent in fire socializafion of children: An historical overview. mvolopmontl Psycholpgy, 28(6), 1006-1017. Mathijssen, J. J. P., Koot, H. M., Verhulst, F. C., DaBruyn, E., 8 Oud, J. H. (1998). The relafionship between mutual family relafions and child psychopafirology. Journal of Child Psycholpgy 8 Psychiagy 8 Allied Disciplines, 39(4), 477-487. May, D. C. 8 Kundert, D. K. (1997). School readiness pracfices and children at-risk: Examining fire issues. Psycholpgy in fire Schmls, 34 (2), 73- 84. 124 McLoyd, V. C., 8 Ceballo, R. (1998). Concaptualizing and assessing economic context: Issues in fire study of race and child development. In V. C. McLoyd 8 L. Steinberg (Eds.), Studying minomy' adolescent: Conceptual, methodological, and fireorefical issues (pp. 251-278). Mahwah NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. Minuchln, S. (1992). The rostoriod history of family firerapy. In J. K. Zeig (Ed.), The avolufion of gychotherapy: The second conference (pp. 3- 12). Philadelphia: BrunnerlMazel, Inc. Moffitt, T. E. (1990). Juvenile delinquency and attenfion deficit disorder: Boys’ developmentl trajectories from age 3 to age 15. Child Development, 61, 893-910. Morrison, J. W., Ipsa, J., 8 Milner, V. (1998). Ideas about child rearing among Jamaican mofirers and early childhood educafion teachers. Journal of Research in Childhood Educafion 12(2), 166-175. Morrison, J. W., 8 Milner, V. (1995). Formal oducafion of children in Jamaica. Childhood Educafion 71 194-196. Murphy-Bonnan, V., Levesque, H. L., 8 Barman, J. J. (1996). U.N. convonfion on the right of fire child: A cross-cultural view (1996). American Psychologist 51 (12), 1257-1261. Nelson, K. (1996). Language in cpgnifive development: Emergence of fire mediated mind. New York: Cambridge University Press. Olson, D. H., 8 Russell, C. S. (1980). Circumplex model of maritl and family systems II: Empirical studies and clinical intervenfion. Advances In Family lntervonfion, Assessment, and Theory, 1, 129-179. Olson, D. H., Sprenklo, D. H., 8 Russell, C. S. (1979). Circumplex model of maritl and family systems: I. Cohesion and adaptbility dimensions, family types, and clinical applicafions. Family Processes, 18, 3-28. Pedhazur, E. J. (1973). Mulfiple mression in behavioral resoarch:Explanafion and predicfion (2nd ed.). Chicago: Holt, Rinehart and Winston. Plant, R. C. (1999). Enhancing rolafionshlg between children and teachers. Washington DC: American Psychological Associafion. 125 Portes, P. R., Cuents, T. E., 8 Zady, M. (2000). Cognifivo socializafion across efirnocultural context: literacy and cultural differences in intellectual performance and parent-child interacfion. The Journal of Genefic Psychology, 161(1), 79-98. Portes, P. R., Sandhu, D. S., Cuents, T. E., 8 Zady, M. (1995). Cultural and school achievement aspect of parent-child interacfions. Korean Journalof Thinking 8 Problem-Solving, 5(2), 21 -44. Quigglo, N. L., Garber, J., Panak, W. F., 8 Dodge, K. A. (1992). Social informafion processing in aggressive and depressed children. Child Developmenp 63, 1305-1320. Radke-Yarrow, M. (1998). Children of depressed mofirels: From early childhood to maturity. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. 216 pp. Ramsey, P. G. (1988). Social skills and peer sttus: A comparison of two socioeconomic groups. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 34(2), 185-202. Raver, C. C., 8 Leadbeater, B. J. (1999). Mofirering under pressure: Environmentl, child, and dyadic correlats of maternal self-efficacy among low-income women. Journal of Family Psycholpgy, 13(4), 523- 534. Richards, M. P. (1974). The int_egration of a child into a social world. New York: Cambridge University Press. Rodgers-Jenkins, F. 8 Chapman, D. W. (1990). Job safisfacfion of Jamaican elementry school teachers. International Review of Educafionllntemafionale Zeitcrift fur Erziehungswissenschafthevuo lnternafionale do po_dagpgie, 36 (3), 299-313. Rogler, L. H. (1999). Mefirodological sources of culfilral insensifivity in mantl healfir research. American Psycholpgist, 54(6), 424-433. Rogoff, B. (1990). Apprenficeship in firinking: anifive development in a social context. New York: Oxford University Press. Rogoff, B., Baker-Sonnett, J., Lacasa, P., 8 Goldsmifir, D. F. (1995). Development firrough parficipafion in sociocultural acfivity. In J. Goodnow 8 P. Miller (Eds.), Cultural pracfices as context for development (pp. 45-65). 126 Rogoff, B., Mistry, J., Goncu, A., 8 Mosier, C. (1993). Guided parficipafion in cultural acfivity by toddlers and caregivers. Monographs of fire Society for Research in Child Development, 58(8), v-179. Rose, D. (1998). Alexithymia and hosfility in Jamaican adult. Unpublished Masters Thesis, Michigan Stte University, E. Lansing. Rofirbaum, F., 8 Weisz, J. (1994). Parentl caregiving and child externalizing behavior in nonclinical samples: A met-analysis. Psycholpgical Bulletin, 116(1), 55-74. Rutter, M. (1995). Relafionships betrneon mantl disorders in childhood and adulfilood. Act Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 91(2), 73-85. Rutter, M., Yule, B., Morton, J., a Bagley, c. (1975). Children of West Indian immigrant: III. Home circumstnces and family patterns. Journal of Child Psycholpgy 8 Psychiatry 8 Allied Disciplines, 16(2), 105-123. Sameroff, A. J. (1975). Transacfional models in early social relafions. Human Development, 18(1-2), 65-79. Sameroff, A. J., 8 Seifer, R. (1990). Early contributors to developmentl risk. In J. Rolf 8 A. S. Masten (Eds.), Risk and protecfivo factors in fire development of psychopafirolpgy (pp. 554). New York: Cambridge University Press. Sameroff, A. J., 8 Seifer, R. (1995). Accumulafion of environmentl risk and child mantl healfir. In H. E. Fitgerald 8 B. M. Lester (Eds.), Children of mveLty: Research, healfir, and alley issues (pp. 233-258). New York: Garland Publishing, Inc. Samms-Vaughan, M. E. (1998a). Preliminag scienfific reflrt on fire relafionship between iron deficiency anaemia, mantl development and behavior in Jamaican pro-school children: University of fire West Indies. Samms-Vaughan, M. E. (1998b). An epidmiological anajgis of cpgnifion, educafional attainment and behaviour in a cohort of Jamaican children: University of fire West Indies. Satfier, J. (1992). Assessment of children: Revised and ugated firird edifion. San Diego: Jerome Satfier, Publisher, Inc. 127 Scott-McDonald, K. (1997). The sttus of child care support for Jamaican families. In J. Roopnarina 8 J. Brown (Eds.), Caribbean families: Diversity among efirnic groug (pp. 147-176). Greenwich, CT: Ablex Publishing Corporafion. Seifer, R., 8 Sameroff, A. J. (1987). Mulfiple determinant of risk and invulnerability. In E. J. Anfirony 8 B. J. Cohler (Eds.), fills; invulnerable child (pp. 51 -69). New York: The Guilford Press. Sroufe, L. A., 8 Fleeson, J. (1988). The coherence of family relafionships. In R. Hinde 8 J. Stevenson-Hinde (Eds.), Rolafionshig wifirin familipg: Mutual influences (pp. 2747). Oxford: Clarendon Press. Stbbins, R. A. (1973). Physical context influences behavior: The case of classroom disorderlinoss. Environment and Behavior, 5 (3), 291 -314. Sfipek, D. J. (2001). Pafirways to construcfive lives: The importance of early school success. In A. C. Bohart8 D. J. Stipek (Eds), Construcfive. I d (pp. 291-316). Washington, D. C.: American Psychological Society. Sfipok, D. J., 8 Ryan, R. H. (1997). Economically disadvantaged preschoolers: Ready to loam but furfiler to go. Developmentl Psycholpgy, 33(4), 71 1 -723. Storrnont, M. (1998). Family factors associated wifir externalizing disorders in preschoolers. Journal of Early lntervenfion, 21 (3), 232-251. Stonnshak, E., Bionnan, K., and The Conduct Problems Prevonfion Research Group (1998). The implicafions of different developmentl patterns of disrupfive behavior problems for school adjustment. Development and Psychogfirolpgy, 10, 451-467. Tamowski, K. J., 8 Rohrbeck, C. A. (1993). Disadvantged Children and Families. In T. H. Ollendick 8 R. J. Prinz (Eds.), Advances in Clinical Child Psycholpgy (Vol. 15, pp. 41 -78). New York: Plenum Press. Tefi, D. M., Gelfand, D., Messinger, D. S., 8 Isabella, R. (1995). Maternal depression and fire quality of early attchmont: An examinafion of infant, preschoolers and fireir mofirers. Developmentl Psycholpgy, 31(3), 364-376. Tremblay, R. E. (1999). When children's social development fails. In D. Kaafing 8 C. Hartman (Eds.), Developmentl healfir and fire wealfir of nafions: Social, biological, and educafional dynamics (pp. 55-71). New York: The Guilford Press. 128 Valsiner, J. (1989). Human Development and Culture: The Social Nature of Personality and It Study (First ed.). Lexington: D.C. Hoafir and Company. Vygotky, L. S. (1930l1978). Mind in society: The development of higher gycholpgical processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Weissman, M., 8 Warner, V. (1997). Offspring of depressed parent: 10 years later. Archives of General Psychiat_ry, 54, 932-940. Weissman, M. M., Gammon, G. D., John, K., 8 Morikangas, K. R. (1987). Children of depressed parent: Increased psychopafirology and early onset of major depression. Archives of General Psychiat_ry, 44(10), 847-853. . Whit, B. L. (1995). The new first years of life. New York: Fireside Book. Wint, E., 8 Brown, J. (1987). Promofing effecfivo parenfing: a study of two mofirods in Kingston, Jamaica. Child Welfare, 66, 507-516. Zahn-Waxlor, C., Cole, P. M., Richardson, D. T., Friedman, R. J., Michel, M. K., 8 Belouad, F. (1994). Social problem solving in disrupfive preschool children: Reacfions to hypofirefical situafions of conflict and distress. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 40(1), 98-119. Zucker, R. A., 8 Fitgerald, H. E. (1991). Early developmentl factors and risk for alcohol problems. Alcohol Healfir and Research Worlg, 15(1), 18-24. Zucker, R. A., 8 Gomberg, E. S. (1986). Efiology of alcholism reconsidered: The case for a biopsychosocial process. American Psycholpgist, ,4_1_ (7), 783-793. 129 11111111211111 3 011111020: 5 -