: LCV l ‘ .. I. «r», , , wwumunnfirfiu Emma. ‘ . . a". V . . .méfi r: . i S . . . ‘ a? . a. ii i 2., ‘ 34:! . . . unu’nwrnfl was» , i..- 25. - 1A .1 .33”: 1%.... u . 5.1.21.3 .unwflw . :15...) (I! .1... . r .13..-.tqusrq . n. : . , a. Iflki: r: If. «9:! mm. 2... 1.3.2.? 5.3.3.. 1.0.”. 3. x I. II “fl ”WWW . . . .16.. ., “a... .. H . :5: Sigma”. . N $3?” a. a??? ., n .._u 44 EL: ., r ., .. .. _._.. m .F. m, “aw... i. an. ThEbLS DOOl This is to certify that the thesis entitled An fMRI Study of Regular and Irregular Inflections in German presented by Carrie Lynn Campbell has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for M.A. . Linguistics degree in [jewel Major professor Date XfiQlflO] 0-7639 MS U is an Affirmative Action/Equal Opportunity Institution LIBRARY Michigan State University PLACE IN RETURN BOX to remove this checkout from your record. TO AVOID FINES return on or before date due. MAY BE RECALLED with earlier due date if requested. DATE DUE DATE DUE DATE DUE 6/01 c:/CIRC/DateDue.p65-p.15 AN FMRI STUDY OF REGULAR AND IRREGULAR INFLECTIONS IN GERMAN BY Carrie Lynn Campbell AN ABSTRACT OF A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF ARTS Department of Linguistics 2001 Associate Professor Alan Beretta ABSTRACT AN FMRI STUDY OF REGULAR AND IRREGULAR INFLECTIONS IN GERMAN By Carrie Lynn Campbell Theories about the processing of regular and irregular inflections have disagreed over whether they should be represented by a dual or single component model. Traditional linguistic theory assumes the two components of grammar for regular inflections and memory for regular stems and irregular inflections. Rumelhart and McClelland (1986) have introduced a computer model which dispenses with symbolic rules and seeks to explain both regular and irregular inflections with a single mechanism. Event-related fMRI was used to test this distinction on German inflections. Plural formation of regular and irregular nouns and past participle formation of regular and two different classes of irregular verbs were studied. These theories have different implications for neural activation. lithe two-component hypothesis reflects the actual organization of the language faculty, two distinct patterns of brain activation should be observed for production of irregular and regular noun and verb inflections. If a single-route model is correct, then there should be no neural dichotomy between German regular and irregular inflection. These results show distinct patterns for neural activation for regulars and irregular inflection in the total level of activation, hemispheric balance of activation, and specific regions activated by each inflectional class. These findings are consistent with predictions of the two-component model of inflection. ACKNOWLDGEMENTS The author is grateful to the members of her committee, Alan Beretta (chair), Tom Carr, and Yen-Hwei Lin; to Yue Cao and Jie Huang whose direction and guidance was crucial to this project; to David McFarlane for training on E-Prime programming; and to Kiel Christianson and Lothar Schmitt for helping with the word frequencycounts. She acknowledges also the generous support of Jim Potchen and the Radiology Department of Michigan State University for fostering linguistic fMRI research at Michigan State University. This research was funded in part by a summer graduate assistantship from the Cognitive Science Program at Michigan State University. iii TABLE OF CONTENTS List of Tables ...................................................................................................... viii List of Figures ...................................................................................................... vii 1 Introduction .................................................................................................... 1 1.1 The challenge: connectionism ................................................................ 2 1.1.1 Outline of the model ......................................................................... 2 1.1.2 The ensuing debate ......................................................................... 3 1.2 The response: dual routes 5 1.2.1 The default nature of the regular ...................................................... 7 1.2.2 Frequency effects ............................................................................ 8 1.2.3 Morphological priming ...................................................................... 9 1.2.4 Aphasia and impaired systems ...................................................... 10 1.2.5 Acquisition ..................................................................................... 12 1.3 Counterarguments ................................................................................ 13 1.3.1 Successes of connectionism ......................................................... 13 1.3.2 lndecisive neuroimaging studies .................................................... 14 1.3.3 Limitations of English ..................................................................... 19 1.4 German ................................................................................................. 20 1.4.1 Facts of the German system .......................................................... 21 1.4.2 Frequency ...................................................................................... 23 1.4.3 The case for default in German ..................................................... 23 1.4.4 Frequency effects .......................................................................... 27 iv 1.4.5 Priming .......................................................................................... 28 1.4.6 Aphasia .......................................................................................... 29 1.5 Neuroimaging ....................................................................................... 29 1.5.1 Event-related brain potentials (ERP) ............................................. 29 1.5.2 PET ................................................................................................ 30 1.6 Overview of the current study ............................................................... 31 2 Methods ....................................................................................................... 33 2.1 Subjects ................................................................................................ 33 2.2 Materials ............................................................................................... 33 2.3 Language Protocol .................................................. g .............................. 34 2.4 MR Imaging Protocol ............................................................................ 35 2.5 Image Processing ................................................................................. 36 3 Results ......................................................................................................... 39 3.1 Empirical Observations ......................................................................... 39 3.1.1 Common Map ................................................................................ 39 3.1.2 l-R Contrast ................................................................................... 40 3.1.3 R-l Contrast ................................................................................... 41 3.2 Regularity .............................................................................................. 43 3.3 Regional Analysis ................................................................................. 45 3.4 Lateralization ........................................................................................ 45 4 Discussion .................................................................................................... 46 4.1 Levels of activation ............................................................................... 46 4.2 Hemispheric differences ....................................................................... 47 4.3 Regions of activation ............................................................................. 47 5 Conclusion ................................................................................................... 51 REFERENCES .................................................................................................... 52 APPENDIX .......................................................................................................... 57 1. Verbs ........................................................................................................ 57 2. Nouns ....................................................................................................... 59 vi LIST OF TABLES Average activation in brain regions common to both regular and irregular data .40 Average activation of brain regions in the l-R subtraction condition .................... 41 Average activation of brain regions in the R-l subtraction condition .................... 42 Areas of activation for regulars and irregulars in previous neuroimaging studies compared with results from the current study ..................................................... 48 vii LIST OF FIGURES Left and right hemisphere contrasts between regular and irregular activation in a single subject ..................................................................................................... 43 Regular and irregular paradigms in the left hemisphere of two subjects ............ 44 viii 1 Introduction Traditionally the language faculty has been viewed as a set of stored, memorized words in addition to a set of combinatorial rules to help put the stored elements together. This system relies both on rules that manipulate symbols, or variables, allowing different stored words to utilize the same set of rules. This system has recently been challenged by those who would model language acquisition using only a single process of associative memory. The debate between these two views has focused primarily on inflectional morphology. Symbol-manipulators argue that two routes are necessary to process inflections: memory stores individual lexical stems and irregular inflected items while regular inflections require an additional rule that is applied either to an existing stem or extended to other stems. However, in an effort to posit as simple a system as possible, some modelers have eliminated the rule-based route and attempt to show that human-like performance can be achieved based on memory processes alone. Henderson has written (1965), “Research into the activity of the brain is, of course, considerably handicapped by its almost complete inaccessibility to objective investigation. It has then to be studied indirectly through the behavior which results from it; and of all kinds of behavior linguistic behavior is one of the most revealing for this purpose.” The rules studied by modern linguists have been meant to do precisely what Henderson suggested; dual-route accounts of inflection assume that the rules linguists have formulated based on behavior truly do reflect the activity of the brain. Single-route theories view them merely as descriptive of behavior and not the underlying processes. A great deal of evidence has been amassed that supports the notion of a default regular inflection but, at the same time, associative models have been devised which their proponents believe are clear advances over earlier models. This study uses neuroimaging to “ask the brain” how many systems it uses to process inflection. 1.1 The challenge: connectionism The event sparking this controversy was a computer model of language acquisition and processing described by Rumelhart and McClelland (1986). This model, consisting of a network of densely connected units representing features in input and output forms, used probabilistically weighted connections which “learned” to associate input with output forms by adjusting connection strengths and thresholds. 1.1.1 Outline of the model This model employs a distributed network, where each input and output node corresponds not to a particular word, but to a feature, or in this case a set of features. Because Rumelhart and McClelland use Wickelphones, or sets of three adjacent phonemes, to break up words, they employ sets of simplified features for three adjacent phones in their representation (Wickelfeatures). Each node codes for a triple of features, one each from the predecessor, central, and successor phoneme. Only combinations of different values on the same dimension for the predecessor and successor phonemes, as well as those encoding word-boundaries, were used, giving a total of 460 Wickelfeatures. Thus two sets of 460 nodes were necessary to encode both inputs and outputs. Node activations are discrete, so units that are on to represent the word should be set to 1 and all other set to O. The threshold for the activation of each unit is established through the training procedure. Connections between these nodes are initially set to 0 so that the input nodes have no influence on the output. During the learning stage, the model is presented with both the root form of a verb and the correct target. lt computes, based on the strengths of its connections, an output given the verb root input, and compares this with the actual target. The connection strengths are then adjusted using a variation of the delta rule which adjusts both the connection strength and the threshold value by a value of one when the output of a node does not match the target output. Rumelhart and McClelland trained their model to perform English past tense inflection by training it on sets of words which arguably reflected the input a child would receive: early training took place on a set of ten high-frequency verbs of which 8 were irregular and later training stages involved progressively lower frequency (and more predominantly regular) verbs. The model was able to successfully replicate some observed patterns in child language acquisition, including a U-shaped learning curve with stages of accurate but limited performance, overregularization, and proper usage. 1.1.2 The ensuing debate Based on this success, Rumelhart and McClelland declared, “We have shown that a reasonable account of the acquisition of past tense can be provided without recourse to the notion of a ‘rule’ as anything more than a description of the language. We have shown that, for this case, there is no induction problem. The child need not figure out what the rules are, nor even that there are rules” (p. 267, emphasis theirs). This assurance that rules would be henceforth unnecessary and challenged the very foundations of modern linguistics and cognitive science which both rely on symbol manipulation. Because the notion of rules involving some kind of symbol or variable is fundamental to classical cognitive science and linguistics, this challenge has sparked much debate. The debate, however, need not be between networking models themselves and those who insist on the necessity of some rule involving symbolic manipulation. As Marcus (2001) shows, models built using connectionist networks can in fact make use of rule-and-memory systems, whether explicitly or inadvertently. Westermann and Goebel (1995), for example, purposely include a short—term memory module to represent the regular, rule- based path as well as a phonological lexicon for irregulars. Hare et al. (1995) have a similar two-part network including one feedforward network that works like othersuch models, without implementing any rules, and a second “clean-up” network, which copies the stem automatically, preparing it for the application of a rule. This effectively implements symbolist suggestions about possible mechanisms for rule-and-memory models: it computes a possible output based purely on phonological associations, then passes this output and the verb stem along to the clean-up network. If the feedforward network’s output is strongly activated, the clean-up network is suppressed, but if weakly activated the clean- up network is engaged, applying the —ed suffixation. Thus “connectionist” is not a term which necessarily implies an advocate of alternatives to symbol manipulation. However, because most connectionist networks under study currently are in fact advocating such alternatives, the term connectionist will be used in this paper in that somewhat over-simplified sense. 1.2 The response: dual routes The first major reply to this connectionist model and related models came from Pinker and Prince (1988). They propose that neither a totally rule-based nor a totally associative system can account for the acquisition of regular and irregular forms, but suggest instead an associative process of acquisition and access for irregular forms and a rule-based process for regulars used for default production. In response to the connectionist challenge they point out certain facts about English regular and irregular morphology that any model must be able to explain to be considered successful and discuss the inability of the Rumelhart and McClelland model and any that rely on a single associative process to address these issues. The notion of a default is crucial to the dual-route approach. While in English the concepts of the regular as both a default and common ending are confounded, the generalizability of certain forms, not their prevalence by token or type, is the considered crucial characteristic of a default regular in this account. Irregular inflections apply in a limited way to certain classes of words (which may be large or small) and should never be generalized to words which have no similarity to their class. Regular inflections, however, should be able to apply to all words not included in an irregular class, and may even be used on novel words which resemble irregulars or be misapplied to words in an irregular class, thus intruding into “irregular territory”. This default nature of the regular is a major defense of Pinker and Prince against associative models. For human language shows that regular endings can apply not only in circumstances in which the word to be inflected resembles other regular words (this would be accounted for easily by connectionist networks), but also when it resembles or is even a homophone to an irregular word. Given the association of a phonological form with an irregular inflection already, such a system should not consistently regularize these regular homophones of irregular words. This pattern of challenge to connectionist models has been elaborated by others as well, notably by Marcus et al. (1995) who synthesized Pinker and Prince’s list with other subsequent evidence found for the distribution of regulars. They find at least 21 distinct circumstances in which regular inflections are applied as a default, including many that connectionist models have even yet not been able to account for. One major difficulty with current connectionist’models is their reliance on a purely phonological (or Wickelphonological) representation of the input which is inconsistent with the features of real language. Even while all subsequent models have abandoned Wickelphones, the reliance on mere phonological input in almost all current models ignores other aspects of words such as semantics and does not reflect human language use. Such a system cannot account for . homophonous words which differ in regularity (ring a bell/ring a campfire) or forms similar to large classes of irregulars that are regular themselves (throw- threw vs. flow-flowed). Large classes of nouns that consistently take regular inflections include members with phonological similarity to or identity with irregular forms. These words show not only the limitations of the phonological reliance of connectionist systems, but the generalizability of regular inflection. Furthermore, phonology-only inputs have no basis for recognizing the stem within the input because phonetic distinctions often fail to distinguish the stem from its affixes. This explains the many recent models that show more blends (e.g. wented) than overregularizations (e.g. Plunkett and Juola 1999, Plunkett and Marchman 1991, MacWhinney and Leinbach 1991) — contrary to the facts of child development (Marcus et al. 1992). 1.2.1 The default nature of the regular Extensive lists of verb classes that are consistently regularized are seen throughout the literature (see Pinker and Prince 1988, Marcus et al. 1995). The most prominent and robust are discussed below. First, words which lack an entry in memory or have no similar entry from which to draw analogies are consistently regularized. This class includes novel words (snarfed), low frequency words which may not have a participle entry (stinted), and unusual nonce words (ploamphed). While connectionist models built upon systems like English with regular forms in the majority can generalize to new forms accurately, forms which are completely unrelated to forms recognized by the model cause trouble for connectionist models (Seidenberg 1992). Words whose linguistic entry is not a canonical root including the full complement of phonological, syntactic, and semantic information are inflected according to the regular paradigm. Such words include onomatopoeia (peeped), quotations (“man”’s), names (the Chi/d3), some borrowings (kimonos), and acronyms (CEOs). Many of these forms bear great similarity to irregular forms and thus risk irregularization by analogy in a connectionist model. Other words which are derived from a different category share a weakness in mental representation because they cannot be marked for the proper inflectional features. Such denominal verbs (high-sticked), deadjectival verbs (n'ghted the boat), and nominalizations (wo/fs of food) are regularized and cannot be accounted for using purely phonological features. In cases of headless compounds, features from the head cannot percolate to the whole word because they differ in category. This occurs often in compounds (Iow-lifes) or derivations via a name (Toronto Maple Leafs). 1.2.2 Frequency effects Frequency effects for irregular participles show that lower frequency irregulars are significantly more likely to be overregularized than high frequency irregulars. Frequency is not an effect, however, for error rates in regular participles (Pinker and Prince 1991). This is in keeping with an account in which regulars involve a rule which is unaffected by frequency, while irregulars rely on memory, which would be less reliable for low than for high frequency items. Furthermore, frequency comes into play in response times. Prasada et al. (1990) show distinctions between performance on regulars and irregulars when subjects orally produced past tense forms from visually presented infinitives of the same verbs. High frequency irregulars were significantly faster than low frequency irregulars, while regulars showed no such frequency effect. Again, this fits most clearly into a dual-route account of inflection. 1.2.3 Morphological priming Priming experiments present subjects with two stimuli while their relationship is manipulated to determine the effect that processing the first stimulus will have on processing the second. Repetition of a stimulus facilitates its lexical access, so the second in a set of identical stimuli is generally associated with faster response times for tasks performed with the stimulus. ERP effects are also associated with primed stimuli. In a study of visual priming, Miinte et al. (1999) showed that regular past participles in English exhibited a priming effect on their stems while irregulars did not. ERPs to regular verbs were associated with an N400 reduction in the primed condition, while ERPs to irregulars showed no such effect. This study was controlled for phonological and orthographic priming, so the effect observed is likely due to regularity and not merely to formal similarity of participles to their stems. The priming of regular participles by their infinitives and vice versa is consistent with the dual-route model. Because regular participles are made up of stem and affix, processing either form will activate the lexical entry for the stem. Because the presentation of the first instance of the verb accesses the stem, the second instance will be facilitated in stem access, even if the inflectional aspects of the word are different. Irregular participles and infinitives, however, are separate lexical entries under the dual-route model, and so priming should not occur as the above studies show. 1.2.4 Aphasia and impaired systems 1.2.4.1 Aphasia ln agrammatic Broca’s aphasia inflectional morphology is often impaired, presumably by damage to the part(s) of the brain which are responsible for the processing of inflection. By observing the patterns of breakdown one can compare the major theories of inflection because these models should not be equally vulnerable to certain types of impairment. lithe dual-route model is correct, then it should be possible to selectively impair either regular or irregular inflection in a language disorder. Marslen-Wilson and Tyler (1997) describe just such a double—dissociation in impairment. They tested three agrammatic aphasic subjects on an auditory priming task in which subjects were asked to decide whether the spoken target word (following a spoken prime) was a word or not. Morphological priming effects for inflected regular and irregular primes as well as semantic priming effects were measured. Control subjects showed priming in all three of these cases, with no interaction between regularity and priming. However, the aphasic patients showed a three-way interaction between patient, regularity, and priming, with two aphasic patients showing priming for irregular past tense and inhibition for regular past tense and one aphasic producing the opposite pattern. 10 Japanese aphasics have also been studied and have been shown to exhibit a dissociation by aphasia type in preference for selecting a regular or irregular nominal suffix in sample discourses (Hagiwara et al. 1999). Broca’s aphasics selected a regular nominal ending when it was semantically required significantly less often than did normals or aphasics with other damage. Gogi aphasics, however, who are characterized by intact inflection but impaired lexical retrieval, used significantly more regular nominalizations than did normals in a context that forced irregular suffixation. While studies in connectionism have succeeded in impairing regular performance by removing nodes from a model, creating a “lesion”, none have replicated an impaired irregular system by this process. Although this does not show decisively that lesions to an associative system could not produce the above patterns, the “double dissociation” of the aphasia studies described above reflects more clearly the dual-route analysis which allows for selective impairment of either route. 1.2.4.2 Alzheimer’s disease In a variation on Pinker’s (1991) dual-route theory, Ullman has advanced a theory that the lexicon is part of a “declarative/semantic memory” system that includes other factual memory and that grammatical rules are stored in a “procedural/skill system” that also handles some non-linguistic skills. He bases much of his theory on contrasts among Alzheimer’s patients, who he finds to be impaired in tasks in his “declarative memory” category (e.g. irregular production and animal naming) but relatively spared in “procedural” tasks (e.g. past tense ll production and tool naming), and Parkinson’s Disease, which shows the opposite pattern (Ullman et al. 1997, Ullman1999). Thus, while Ullman does not suggest that language areas in the brain are specialized for language alone, he supports the idea that rules and memory are distinct components of the system. He even suggests different neuroanatomical locations for them related to areas of damage in each of these diseases, identifying the impairment of declarative and lexical memory with damage to the tempoparietal cortex and that of procedural or grammatical rules with damage to the frontal/basaI-ganglia system including Broca’s area. 1.2.5 Acquisition Given the evidence described above, there are solid grounds for claiming that adults make a distinction between a default regular and a structured lexical entry for irregulars. It seems logical, as well, to ask how this distinction came to be, and whether the patterns of acquisition can give any hints about the processing of inflection. On one level, it is quite impressive that children can possibly learn that one inflection is a default, since irregular words make up a majority of the high- frequency children encounter early on in acquisition. Furthermore, inflections are non-salient, being monosyllabic (or less) and spoken as unstressed bound morphemes. Eventually, it seems, children must notice that only regulars can apply to any phonological pattern and in unusual circumstances (names, borrowings, onomatopoeia, etc). Since this observation relies on experience, it 12 may take children some time to fully identify and grasp the default in any given language. However, research has shown that children are in fact quite adept at identifying defaults in English; by age four they use the -ed past tense for novel words. In fact, children characteristically overapply the default, in overregularization errors like holded (Marcus et al. 1992). Yet these overregularizations actually seem to follow certain patterns, showing sensitivity to frequency and similarity (fewer errors take place on high-frequency or large- family irregulars), and ultimately accounting for only 10% of all inflections. Children also reflect adult behavior in compounding words, allowing irregular but not regular plurals in compounds like rat-eater (not rats-eater, but mice-eater is okay). In all these respects children’s behavior mirrors that of adults and make clear the need for any successful model of language acquisition to be able to recognize and properly apply default inflections. 1.3 Counterarguments Given all the data above that are consistent with a dual-route account of inflection and pose challenges to connectionism, why has eliminative connectionism not yet been eliminated? 1.3.1 Successes of connectionism Since Rumelhart and McClelland’s 1986 model, many new models have been developed which have succeeded in dealing with the individual problems pointed out by Pinker and Prince. For example, MacWhinney and Leinbach (1991) describe a model that can account for the treatment of homonyms to 13 irregulars as regular, but it doesn’t succeed in consistently adding —ed to unusual-sounding, unfamiliar words or in avoiding blends as real speakers do. Plunkett and Juola (1999) present a model that properly shows frequency effects for both response time and error rate, but it fails to avoid blends and the authors do not demonstrate clearly whether the model can generalize —ed to unusual- sounding unfamiliar words. Thus these new models are often very good at the particular problem for which they are tailored, but still fail to demonstrate how a comprehensive single- route connectionist model can account for all the various facts of English inflection. Yet each new partial success serves to keep the controversy alive. Furthermore, neuroimaging research to date has been inadequate and difficult to interpret as described above. First, the use of block design makes it unclear what is being tested. Additionally, many studies (and all discussed thus far) have made use of English, which is not an ideal language to decide between the competing accounts of inflection. 1.3.2 lndecisive neuroimaging studies Seidenberg writes that “The nature of language is determined by how it is acquired and used and therefore needs to be explained in terms of these functions and the brain mechanisms that support them.” (1997, p. 1601) If the goal of each both associationists and symbolists is to describe the underlying brain mechanisms, then a useful area to search for answers in this debate is that of neuroimaging, in effect asking the brain how many different processes it thinks it has. In this section several recent neuroimaging techniques 14 and findings pertaining to this debate will be reviewed. However, because of several confounds of design and findings that are difficult to interpret make the data obtained in these studies indecisive as regards the symbolist/connectionist debate. 1.3.2.1 Positron emission tomography (PET) PET measures metabolic activity in the brain by detecting positrons emitted from an isotope ingested by the subject. Scans acquired in this way can be used to provide estimates of regional cerebral blood flow (rCBF). One major drawback to this method of neuroimaging is its inability to look at real-time stimuli and requirement for block design. Using this technique, Jaeger et al. (1996) found that both regular and irregular English verbs activate the left middle and inferior frontal gyri (including Broca’s area) and bilaterally activate the precuneus. Regular verbs in their study were associated with left dorsolateral prefrontal, left anterior cingulate, and anterior left inferior parietal activation, while irregular verbs activated the left superior frontal gyrus, left superior parietal Iobule, posterior left inferior parietal Iobule and left middle temporal gyrus. However, this study used a block design that may have resulted in subjects forming response strategies to the regular and irregular conditions. Penke et al. (1997) note that, given the block design of the study, it is impossible to know whether the observed activity is truly due to a regular-irregular distinction or merely the production of different kinds of lists. In their critique of Jaeger et al., Seidenberg and Hoeffner (1998) note several ways in which block design confuses the issue actually being tested. 15 First, the large body of priming research already demonstrates that performance on one trial of an experiment is affected by its similarity or dissimilarity to preceding trials (see section 1.2.3). Second, if blocks that are predictably made up of regular verbs the subject need not processa word beyond the final phoneme to know which inflection to apply (the regular) and to determine which allomorph of the —ed ending to use. For irregular blocks, generating the correct past tense form would still require the recognition of the word as a particular lexical item. When items are presented randomly all words must be processed equally deeply to determine which inflection to use. Jaeger et al.’s own RT data actually support the claim that their subjects employed a strategy that did not require identifying words in regular blocks as distinct lexical items, but only accessing superficial features. In their study, it took subjects 38 ms on average to produce a regular past tense, while other research suggests that 100 ms is needed to identify a word, and that fixation on a word in text normally lasts 200- 250 ms. Thus it is plausible that Jaeger et al.’s subjects, rather than identifying words in regular blocks and processing them normally, were taking advantage of the fact that all words required the same inflection and merely attended to enough of the word to choose the proper allomorph. 1.3.2.2 Magnetoencephalography (MEG) MEG uses a special detection device to measure time—varying magnetic fields generated naturally by neural activity. It has the advantage of observing brain activity in “real-time” while maintaining detailed spatial resolution. 16 Rhee et al. 1999 used MEG to examine production of past tense verbs and found dipoles in the left temporoparietal region for both regular and irregular verbs with slightly later dipoles in the left frontal regions only for regular verbs. These results are presented as consistent with a dual-route model in which regular verbs stems must be accessed in memory (temporal) and then inflected in the grammar (frontal), while irregulars find an appropriate inflected form in memory, blocking the grammatical rule to create past tense. However, the prediction of these findings is that regulars must always be slower than irregulars, which is contrary to fact (see section 1.2.2). While vague, in terms of localization these findings are somewhat at odds with the PET study described above. Jaeger et al. (1996) found no common activation in the temporoparietal region though some temporal and parietal areas were activated by irregulars and left frontal regions were activated in regulars. However, the block design of the former and the potentially confounding sentence-frame protocol of the latter study make disagreement with their findings a less serious matter. However, the results of Rhee et al. are also confusing in light of RT studies which find the English irregular verbs take significantly more time to inflect than regulars (Jaeger et al. 1996, Seidenberg 1992, Prasada et al. 1990). If regulars are inflected more quickly than irregulars, it is unclear why they would be characterized by a later dipole than irregulars, as Rhee et al. report. 17 1.3.2.3 Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) Functional MRI is a neuroimaging technique sensitive to changes in deoxyhemoglobin in brain tissue. Using powerful magnets to polarize molecules in the body, fMRI detects the variable differences in transverse magnetization decay time (T2) and the local magnetic field homogeneity (T2*) of water in the brain tissues. Because the ration of deoxygenated to oxygenated hemoglobin in the blood in a small region, or voxel, influences the local T2*, detection of T2* signal levels can be used to track blood flow and oxygen balance in tissue. fMRI gives good anatomical resolution, though its temporal resolution is far less than that of ERP. It also allows for an event-related study that avoids the pitfalls of the block design as described above. Furthermore, fMRI has been shown by Pugh et al. (1997) to be predictive of psychological performance in a word identification study. These features make fMRI a good tool to use to investigate the nature of the inflectional system. Ullman et al. (1997) take advantage of this tool in a study which utilized a silent production task. Subjects (all male) were shown verb stems in regular and irregular blocks and were asked to silently produce their past tense forms. Comparisons of both regular and irregular to fixation showed bilateral activation in the inferior frontal gyrus (including Broca’s area), caudate nucleus, superior temporal gyrus, and supramarginal and angular gyri. The frontal cortex and the basal ganglia showed more activation for the irregular than for the regular conditions, while posterior regions did not show this distinction. A decreased signal for irregular compared to fixation was found in the temporal and temporo- parietal areas. This decrease in signal is difficult to interpret and it is not clear 18 how it plays into either a single- or dual-route account of inflections. While areas of differential activation for regulars and irregulars initially suggest distinct processes involved in regular and irregular production, the increased activation in the frontal cortex for irregulars compared to regulars seems to contradict a general association of anterior language areas with syntactic function (e.g. inflectional rules) and posterior areas with memory functions. This study is problematic not only in the interpretation of its findings, but also in its block design. 1.3.3 Limitations of English Two major confounds are present in English which limit its ability to distinguish between single— and dual-route accounts. Regularity interacts with both frequency and affixation so that the characteristics of regular, default forms described above could also be attributed to these differences. 1.3.3.1 Frequency In English, the regular forms of both nouns and verbs are also the most frequent, and thus overregularization could be due to frequency of input and not a default assignment. Plunkett and Marchman (1991, 1993) show that altering the ration of regular to irregular items greatly influences the performance of their network model respecting proper generalization of the regular. Specifically, they find that “generalization is virtually absent when regulars contribute less than 50% of the items overall” (Plunkett and Marchman 1993, p.55). Thus, the prevalence of regulars in English is crucially linked to the ability of connectionist models to represent proper generalization of the regular. 19 1.3.3.2 Affixation English verbal and nominal affixes are similar in that each involves a suffix added to the stem with no stem change. Irregulars, however, are characterized by stem changes for both nouns and verbs, sometimes in addition to other changes (e.g. child-children, dig-dug). While there are some exceptions that have no stem change for the irregular (e.g. fish-fish or let-let), there is no case of simple affixation for irregular without an accompanying stem change. Thus it is possible to argue that the distinctions between regular and irregular described above result at least in part from the difference between processing affixes and stem changes. There is a language which avoids these confounds, and thus provides a better test case for questions about symbolist vs. connectionist systems: German. 1.4 German Although much of the early work that addressed the processing of irregular and regular forms explored English in particular despite the confounds discussed abouve, a wider range of languages is now being considered including Japanese (Hagiwara et al. 1999), Arabic (Wunderlich and Fabri 1995), and Italian (Say and Clahsen in press). Recently German has been suggested as an ideal test case for the issue of inflectional processing leading to a series of studies of this problem in German (Marcus et al. 1995). 20 1.4.1 Facts of the German system 1.4.1.1 Verbs An outline of the German verb paradigm is given in (1). There are three forms for each verb: infinitive, preterite, and participle, listed in that order. lnfinitives are composed of a stern and infinitival suffix —(e)n. Participles are more common than preterites in informal speech, thus they are the inflected form that will be considered here. All participles of the forms considered in this experiment have a ge- prefix; this is necessary phonologically but does not differentiate the morphology and thus will not be considered further. Weak verbs consist of a root and a suffix, -t, strong and mixed verbs may involve stem changes from the infinitive and have suffixes —en and —t, respectively. For theoretical reasons (Wunderlich and Fabri 1995) as well as their default nature (described below), they are considered to be the regular form, while strong and mixed verbs represent different irregular forms. The characterization of the strong irregulars into three classes (shown in (1)) is based on the pattern of stem change associated with each class. (1) a. Regular (“weak”) spiel-en spiel-te ge-spieI-t ‘to play’ ‘played’ ‘(has) played’ b. Irregular (“strong”) ABA (e.g. au-ie-au) lauf-en lief ge-Iauf-en ‘to run' ‘ran’ ‘(has) run’ ABB (e.g. ei-i-r) pfeif-en pfiff ge-pfiff-en ‘to whistle’ ‘whistled’ ‘(has) whistled’ 21 ABC (e.g. eh-ing-ang) geh-en ging ge-gang-en ‘to go’ ‘went’ ‘(has) gone’ c. Irregular (“mixed”) brenn-en brann-te ge-brann-t ‘to burn’ ‘burnt’ ‘(has) burnt’ Thus, unlike those in English, all German participles involve overt affixation. This is crucial for studies contrasting regular and irregular forms because it ensures that findings of regular-irregular difference are not merely the result of differing affixation characteristics. While dual-route theorists would still argue that only the regular verbs have an underlying representation as stern and affix (irregulars being stored in memorY). the forms would naturally be dealt with under one process in single-route models. 1.4.1.2 Nouns German has five plural suffixes: -(e)n, -s, -e, -er, and —0 given in (2). Three of these suffixes are associated with changes on the verb stem as well, but this seems to be due to an independent morphophonemic rule and so will not be considered here. No known systematic pattern adequately explains a relationship between semantic properties and the plural suffix a noun takes; some correlations between gender and morphophonemic properties of the root do exist, but exceptions are extensive. In the case of nouns as for verbs, all formsiconsidered in our experiment have segmentable affixes (no zero inflected nouns are used in this study). 22 (2) a. Regular Auto Auto-s car ‘cars’ b. lrregulars Daumen Daumen ‘thumb’ ‘thumbs’ Hund Hund-e ‘dog’ ‘dogs’ Kind Kind—er ‘child’ ‘children’ Frau Frau-en ‘woman’ ‘women’ 1.4.2 Frequency While English regulars represented the most common class of either nouns or verbs, in German regulars are in the minority, while the remaining forms are split among several irregular classes. Nouns are nearly 100% irregular by type or token, while verbs are less ovenlvhelmingly so. Within the 1000 most common words, the regular verbal ending accounts for 45% of types (Marcus et al. 1995). Thus German avoids the confound of frequency and regularity found in English: defaults cannot be accounted for simply by high frequency. 1.4.3 The case for default in German As discussed in section 1.2, the key feature of a default is its ability to extend to new words regardless of similarity. This included novel words, nonsense words, noncanonical words, unusual—sounding words, etc. The next section will demonstrate that German does indeed have a default inflection for both nouns and verbs that patterns much like the English regular. 1.4.3.1 Extension to nonsense words Clahsen (1997) used the preterite form of nonsense verbs to indicate which regular or irregular class the word should belong to in order to examine potential regularization (or irregularization) of novel words. Subjects were presented with an infinitive and preterite of a nonsense verb in a sentence context, thus determining the class to which the verb should belong. Their task is to use the preterite form of this verb to fill a gap in a second sentence and the participle to fill a gap in a third sentence. In the third sentences, 97% of nonsense verbs that patterned like regulars in the example sentence got the regular —t ending, while only 31% of those that patterned like irregulars got the . irregular —n ending. Sixty-nine percent of those exemplified as irregulars were eventually regularized, while only 3% of the “regular” nonces were irregularized. This is a case of massive encroachment by regulars into irregular territory, with almost no reciprocal irregularization. In terms of frequency, the —s inflection is very rare, accounting for no more than 5% of nouns by token or type, so this inflection is by no means regular in terms of commonality. However, it does behave as a default. Marcus et al (1995) presented singular nonsense nouns to subjects in the context of a sentence. These nonsense nouns were taken from lists that either rhymed with existing German irregular nouns or did not rhyme with any German noun. Subjects were asked to rate the possible German plurals of the noun, also presented in sentence form. When the nonsense noun rhymed with an existing irregular, irregular plurals received significantly higher ratings than for non- rhyming nouns. Conversely, regular plurals received significantly higher ratings 24 for non-rhyming nouns than for rhyming ones. Thus irregular inflections were generalized based on similarity only, while regular inflections were generalized to new nouns when no similarity existed. This generalizability where there is no similarity is the key feature of a default. 1.4.3.2 Extension to noncanonical words by adults With respect to noncanonical roots, German also shows a default pattern much like that of English. Marcus et al. (1995) presented new denominal verbs in sentences with the regular —t or the irregular —n ending. Acceptibility ratings showed significantly better acceptance of the —tthan -n ending. This shows that the regular ending is extended even to denominal verbs homophonous with existing irregular verbs, a clear case of the regular encroaching on regular territory. Further, Marcus et al. presented nouns as borrowings or names in a similar task and found that the regular —s plural ending got significantly higher acceptability ratings than did the irregular -n ending. Thus both verbs and nouns with non-canonical roots resort to the default affixation rule. 1.4.3.3 Errors In a study that investigated the effects of purposely using an incorrect form, Clahsen et al. (1997) presented subjects with sets of two stimuli and asked them to decide whether the items were the same or different. Reaction times should correlate with well—formedness: well-formed words should be judged more quickly than ill-formed ones. The sets of stimuli consisted of nonsense verbs which had already been memorized by the subjects as either regular or irregular. 25 Pairs of these verbs were presented in participle form with either a —tor a —n as an affix. While “regular” nonsense verbs were associated with longer RTs for an —n ending (irregularization) than a -t ending (correct), “irregular” nonsense words showed no difference in RTs for either a —n (correct) or a —t (overregularized) ending. This is logical if the —t ending is a default: an irregularization adding -n to a regular word constitutes an affixation error increasing RT, while a default -t affixed to an irregular word is not a true error as the default can apply across the board. 1.4.3.4 Child Acquisition Because of the confounds of English described above, one could account for children’s overregularization patterns to an extent without employing a default. The frequency of the regular in English makes it possible for overregularizations to be based merely on the number of times a child is exposed to the regular ending. Furthermore, studies finding that children use only singular regulars within compounds, dropping the plural —s could be explained in terms of a preference for bare forms rather than a real adult-like use of inflection. However, German-speaking children behave similarly to English-speaking ones, and these counterarguments do not apply to German. Clahsen and Rothweiler (1993), for example, studied spontaneous speech from 1 year 4 months to 3 years 9 months in nine children and found that incorrect participle endings in German constituted only 10% of children’s total participle endings, just as in English. Almost all of these incorrect endings (93%) were overapplications of the default regular —t. German children also have been 26 shown to use —t overwhelmingly for novel words and incorrect participle endings in elicited production (Weyerts and Clahsen 1994, Weyerts 1997). Likewise, with nouns, children predominantly overgeneralize —s, the default ending. Clahsen et al (1996) found that children presented with low- frequency words from all five noun classes overused —s in 58% of their errors (the next most common error accounted for 26% of errors). This is clearly not a usage proportionate to the approximately 5% of nouns the —s plural actually applies to. In the same experiment, children were asked to form a compound noun describing someone who eats the item they had just produced a plural for. Following the adult constraint on the use of regular plurals within compounds, the children omitted the overregularized forms in compounds significantly more than the non-overregularized forms. 1.4.4 Frequency effects Lexical decision times in German also show similar frequency effects to those in English: low frequency irregular nouns and verbs are associated with greater lexical decision times than regulars. Clahsen et al. (1997) describe a study which involved a word/non-word decision task, measuring RTs for visually presented verbs and nouns. Regulars and irregulars were matched for frequency in high and low frequency groups. High frequency irregulars produced faster RTs than low frequency irregulars, while high and low frequency regulars showed no distinction in RT. These data can be explained in terms of a default regular because memory effects that come into play in irregular inflection when searching for the 27 correct participle form but are not needed to find low-frequency regular forms computed by a default rule. 1.4.5 Priming Sonnenstuhl et al. (1999) investigated RTs for cross-modal priming of German nouns and verbs. Primes were presented orally in participle form while targets were a visual presentation of the 1St person singular verb form. The subject was to perform a lexical decision task on the target word. RTs were compared for (I) identical primes and targets, (ll) morphologically related primes and targets (, and (III) semantically and phonologically unrelated primes and targets. Condition (I) represented full priming and (Ill) no priming, so that the experimental condition (ll) could be compared to both baselines. Verbs were either regular or irregular. For regular nouns and verbs, the experimental condition (II) was the same as (l) and significantly faster than (Ill), showing full priming. Irregular nouns and verbs showed (ll) still significantly faster than (Ill), but significantly slower than (I), i.e. partial priming. This full priming for regulars in German is consistent with the claim that that in regulars stem and affix are decomposed. Partial priming with irregulars is consistent with the claim that irregulars are in structured lexical entries so that roots and inflected forms are related more closely than unrelated words, but still do maintain separate entries. If no structure were present in the lexical entries, no priming would be predicted here. Weyerts et al. (1996) have also shown that regular German past participles preceded by their infinitive show distinctive ERP waveforms 28 characteristic of priming by an identical word, while irregulars show no such evidence of priming. 1.4.6 Aphasia In English-speaking aphasics, it is unclear whether a dissociation in performance on regulars and irregulars actually reflects impaired affixation or a true effect of regularity. To test this in German where affixes are present on both regular and irregular forms, Penke et al. (1999) studied eleven German speaking Broca’s aphasics. Of these, eight were found who had unimpaired performance for regular participles but overregularized irregular verbs extensively, demonstrating that aphasia can impair regular or irregular verbs as a class even when both involve affixes. 1.5 Neuroimaging 1.5.1 Event-related brain potentials (ERP) ERP’s are small voltage fluctuations measured from a set of points on the scalp and timed with the presentation of linguistic stimuli. Variables of amplitude, polarity, location, and latency are used to investigate psycholinguistic issues. While this method offers limited spatial resolution, its temporal resolution is very fine. Using ERP, different brain potentials for German noun regularizations than for irregularizations have been found by Weyerts et al. (1997). While regularizations produce a left anterior negativity (LAN) effect, said to be characteristic of morphosyntactic violations, irregularizations are associated with an N400-like effect, which resembles the effect of pronounceable non-words and 29 is associated with semantic violations. This can be explained under a dual-route model where regularizations are the misapplication of a morphological rule adding an affix to a stem present in the lexicon, the irregular base word. Under that model irregularizations are essentially nonce words, because any affixes or stem changes associated with them are not stored in the grammar. Penke et al. (1997) found similar LAN and N400 effects for regularization and irregularization in German verb past participles. A similar dissociation between reactions to incorrect regular and irregular forms was also found by Clahsen (1997). These dissociations are consistent with a dual-route model of inflection but are not explained in single-route theories. If a unitary method is used for processing both regular and irregular words no dissociation is predicted between the effect of over-applying either an irregular or a regular ending. In particular, the similarity of the effects found for both nouns and verbs in these two studies suggests that the differences in regularity of the words is the source of the different effects, and not some accidental features of frequency or word-form. 1.5.2 PET While a PET study of German inflection by lndefrey et al. (1997) avoided the confounds associated with English faced by Jaeger et al. (1996), it still used a block design. Within regular or irregular blocks, a randomized presentation of frames requiring past tense and participle formation was used to prevent the formation of possible response strategies. However, it is unclear whether this innovation completely avoids the confounds of block design or how the introduction of additional syntax, words, and complexity of task affected the 30 results. Thus the twelve areas identified as activated by one set of conditions or the other are in question. Additionally, the finding of no overlap between areas activated in regular or irregular sentence frames seems suspect because all current models of inflection assume at least some memory processes for lexical access must be common to both irregular and regular inflection. 1.6 Overview of the current study This study seeks to overcome some of the problems of earlier research by using event-related fMRI to investigate neural activation of regular and irregular verbs and nouns in German. Because of its high level of irregularity and parallel affixation for regulars and irregulars, German seems a system ideally suited to a connectionist approach. Thus a dichotomy found between regular and irregular forms in German can be more destructive to a single-route theory than such a dichotomy is in English. German speaking subjects were presented visually with a list of regular and irregular verb infinitives and noun singular forms and asked to produce the participle or plural silently. Data were then analyzed to determine whether any difference was present between regular and irregular forms in general. Stimulus words were presented to subjects in isolation. While presentation within a sentence mimics “real-life” generation to some extent and is used by some researchers (lndefrey et al. 1997), such a form introduces too many uncontrolled variables in the extra words to be feasible. This design introduces no other grammatical processes into the stimulus other than the one being studied. The presentation of individual words also makes it possible to identify more precisely the moment at which the linguistic activity 31 being studied is taking place, since fMRI does not have the fine temporal resolution of other imaging techniques such as ERP. An event-related design with regular and irregular words presented in random order was used to ensure that subjects could not use an alternate strategy to complete the task. This is crucial because it ensures that activation is in fact reflecting processing of regulars and irregulars - the issue at hand -— and not some other activity. Functional MRI provides a tool to allow researchers to “ask the brain” how it processes language more directly than previous experiments were able to. While neither major theory, in its general form, makes a specific prediction of what the architecture of the inflectional system in the brain would look like under MRI, they each suggest a different broad form. In general a single-system theory would predict that regular and irregular forms would localize to a specific area of the brain or be similarly diffused throughout cortical areas. In either case, the prediction would be that regular and irregular forms pattern in the same way. This contrasts with the general prediction of a dual-route model, which expects a different distribution of activation for regular and irregular forms. 32 2 Methods 2.1 Subjects Subjects were eleven native speakers of German, six female and five male, recruited from among faculty members and students at Michigan State University. All were righthanded, free of neurological or psychological disorders, and had no previous knowledge of the cognitive issues involved in the study. Handedness was assessed using the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory (Oldfield 1971 ). Written, informed consent was obtained in accordance with protocols approved by the MSU UCRIHS board, lRB # 99-093. Subjects ranged in age from 23-47 years, mean age 29.5. In total, three of these subjects were excluded from our final analysis. One subject was excluded from analysis because he admitted to having fallen asleep during the scan and did not remember several of the target words during a review afterwards. Motion correction analysis also indicated movement suggestive of sleep. The data from a second subject showed artifacts from the equipment that rendered analysis impossible, and a third subject was excluded because noun scans showed virtually no measurable activation. The eight remaining subjects whose data is presented here were four females and four males, age range 24-45, mean age 29. 2.2 Materials One test and one practice word list was developed for nouns and one of each for verbs (see Appendix 2 for the lists of words used). The test lists contained 33 regular and irregular stimulus words matched for token frequency of the inflected form, following Clahsen et al (1997), using the CELEX database (Baayen et al 1995). The noun paradigm included 12 regular and 12 irregular targets, while the verb paradigm included 12 regular targets and 12 each of two irregular classes of verbs. Irregular class A is inflected with the prefix ge- and suffix —en as described above in section 1.4.1.1. Class B irregulars are inflected in the same way but involve an additional vowel change in the stem. Because this is a preliminary study, the two classes of irregular are considered together, though eventually a separate analysis will be made. Nonetheless, under either a single- or a dual-route model both classes should pattern together based on a shared associative process to access irregulars. Practice word lists consisted of 10 words of both regular and various classes of irregular morphology. Because regular and irregular forms pattern similarly at high frequencies (Pinker and Prince 1988), low frequency words were chosen. Words in the noun list all showed a plural frequency no greater than 20 per million and in the verb list no word had a participle frequency greater than 254 per million. While 254 tokens per million is not generally considered a low frequency, this indicates the lowest possible frequency to match regular and irregulars. Thus, the word lists consisted of the lowest set of matched frequency regulars and irregulars possible. 2.3 Language Protocol During the functional MRI scan, subjects performed both a verb and a noun generation task. In the noun task words were presented to the subjects 34 using an event-related paradigm to randomly mix regular and irregular words. A stimulus word from one of the lists described above was shown to the subject on a computer screen within the magnet. Stimulus words were displayed sequentially every 14 seconds for 1 second each in white letters on a black background. Between these items a fixation cross was displayed. The subjects were told to silently generate the plural of each word as rapidly as possible while maintaining accuracy. Each test paradigm was preceded by a practice scan of at least five words to familiarize the subject with the task. Within the test paradigm two dummy words preceded the randomized targets to allow the subject time to adjust to the task. Data collected during presentation of these first two words were discarded in the analysis. The verb task was identical in design and subjects were asked to silently generate the past participle of the word. The order of presentation of the noun and verb trials was counterbalanced across subjects. The paradigms were synchronized with MRI data acquisition using a device, placed on the floor of the magnet room, which detects each RF signal generated by the EPI pulse sequence. This RF signal was sent via an RF filter to the PC computer running the experimental paradigm so that the computer could coordinate timing of stimulus presentation with scan acquisition. 2.4 MR Imaging Protocol Scans were performed on a 1.5 Tesla clinical scanner in the Department of Radiology, Michigan State University. A T1-weighted MRI axial scan provided guidance in locating areas of subjects’ brain anatomy. After auto-shimming, a gradient echo EPI pulse sequence was used to acquire T2*-weighted images (a 64 X 64 matrix over a 24cm field of view (FOV)). During each language paradigm, these pulse sequences acquired 21 (or 19) sagittal sections with 7.0mm thickness, spacing 0.0mm, Flip Angle 90 degree, and TE/TR = 50/2000ms. After the language paradigms were completed 21 (or 19) T1-weighted spin echo images were collected at the same anatomical locations, with TEfl' R = 14/500ms , flip angle 90 degree, FOV = 24cm, matrix 256x192, and slice thickness/spacing = 7.0/0.0mm. These images were used for anatomical orientation during preliminary analysis. A set of 3D spoiled gradient echo T1-weighted MR images Of the whole brain were collected, TE/T R = 3.3/8ms, flip angle 30 degree, slice- thickness/spacing 1.2/0.0mm, 124 slices, matrix size 256x192, and FOV = 24cm. These images provided a three-dimensional anatomical map used for the localization of activated pixels in the final stage of data collection. 2.5 Image Processing Images collected were first corrected for motion by co-registration for both in-plane translational and rotational movement (Cao et al. 1993). No subjects exhibited translational movement and none showed more than 1.46 degrees rotational movement. Each image in the series was registered with the first baseline image by applying a range of test transformations consisting of planar translations and rotations and selecting the transformation yielding the minimum difference in magnitude between the first image and the transformed image. 36 Images associated with regular and irregular forms were sorted out and separately analyzed. After testing, subjects had been questioned about their responses to the targets. When errors occurred in which the subjects’ responses were no longer appropriate to the target’s category (e.g. regularizing an irregular word) the images corresponding to these targets were included in the category corresponding to the subjects’ reported responses. There were six cases of this sort of subject ‘mistake’, five of which had the effect of treating a class-A irregular verb like a class—B irregular and one- of which gave an irregular plural for a regular noun. Statistical analysis of activated pixels was based on a combination of temporal cross-correlation with a reference function and cluster-size thresholding. All pixels were thresholded at a level of cc magnitude equal to 0.44, which had a one-tailed type I error of < 0.01 per pixel. Pixels passing the co threshold were subject to cluster—size thresholding at 23 pixels. The reference function was generated based on the gamma function S(t) = A(t-to)6e’("‘°)’T where A is the amplitude, to the time delay, 6 the rise time of the curve, and T the width. Our reference function was S(t) = (t-1)2'3e““"’°'65, based on activation in the angular and supramarginal gyri expected because of the written lexical processing involved in our task but not under investigation here. Because of the time delay associated with each successive slice as the magnet proceeds from left to right, a delay proportional to this time lapse was added to the function to create in effect one reference function for each sagittal section. 37 In order to explore effects related to regularity alone and not merely to category (noun-verb), noun and verb images were collapsed to obtain an average regular and irregular image for each subject. Subtractions of these images were then performed, creating maps showing the voxels only activated by regular but not irregular words (R-l), irregular but not regular words (l-R), and those voxels activated in both paradigms (Common). The activation images were overlaid on the 3Dspgr images using the research computer software Analysis of Functional Neuroimages (Robert W. Cox, Medical College of Wisconsin 2000). Localization of the activated pixels in cortical regions was computed using this software and Human Brain Anatomy in Computerized Images (Damasio 1995). 38 3 Resuhs These results discuss both the empirical description of areas of activation ' for regular and irregular conditions and a statistical analysis of the extent of acfivafion. 3. 1 Empirical Observations The average results for all cortical areas studied in each of the three contrasts are shown in tables 1, 2, and 3. Because of possible mismatch in activation maps due to subtle head movement, a comparison of activation in the subtraction contrasts (l-R and R-l) and the activation in the common map was made. It was found that the average total activations for the subtraction conditions were considerable in relation to that of the common map: average total l-R activation was 146% that of the common map, R-l 41% that of the common map. This makes it clear that the activation unique to each of the conditions (regularity or irregularity) is considerable and not simply the result of mismatches of a few voxels along the edges of otherwise similar regions. Thus it is safe to assume that, in terms of the area activated, the difference between regular and irregular conditions is real and not an artifact. 3.1.1 Common Map Regions activated by all subjects in the common map were the left premotor area, left postcentral gyrus, left supramarginal gyrus, and right supramarginal gyrus. Other areas activated by a more than half of the subjects were left and right superior, middle and inferior frontal gyri, right premotor area, left and right precentral gyri, left paracentral gyrus, left and right cingulate gyri, 39 left and right angular gyri, left and right superior parietal lobules, left anterior and posterior superior temporal gyrus, left posterior middle temporal gyrus, and right insula. Broca’s area is not found to be consistently activated, though the left posterior superior temporal gyrus (Wernicke’s area) is activated in most subjects. Region Left Right Total Superior frontal gyrus 22.88” 20.13” 43.00" lMiddle frontal gyrus 15.50* 7.75* 23.25" Inferior frontal gyrus 16.88* 10.75“ 27.63* Broca's area 5.63 1.50 7.13" Premotor area 7.75*” 6.00* 13.75““ Precentral gyrus 9.38* 6.25” 15.63 ** Paracentral gyrus 9.50” 4.75 14.25 Cingulate gyrus 7.50* 4.25” 11.75“." Postcentral gyrus 6.63“ 3.25 9.88" Supramarginal gyrus 38.63" 15.63" 54.25" Angular gyrus 9.88* 13.63” 23.50" Superior parietal Iobule 14.50* 18.38* 32.88* Precuneus 5.25* 2.50“ 7.75* Superior Temporal gyrus: Anterior 3.25” 2.00 5.25* Superior Temporal gyrus: Posterior 4.13* 0.88 500* Middle Temporal gyrus: Anterior 0.50 0.25 0.75 Middle Temporal gyrus: Posterior 5.00* 3.50 8.50” Inferior Temporal gyrus: Anterior 0.00 0.75 0.75 Inferior Temporal gyrus: Posterior 6.25 1.63 7.88 Insula 1.25 5.25* 6.50“ Total 190.25 129.00 319.25 Table 1. Average activation in brain regions common to both regular and irregular data. ** activated in all subjects * activated in a majority of the subjects 3.1.2 l-R Contrast The I-R contrast shows more activation overall than the common map, as well as more consistent activation. The lengthy list of the areas activated by all or most subjects is provided in Table 2. While some of the regions which are activated reliably in the l-R contrast overlap with those in the common map, it is important to note that this activation is necessarily different locations within the 40 overlapping regions for the different contrasts because of the subtraction process used to obtain the maps. Region Left Right Total Superior frontal gyrus 29.75" 33.13" 62.88“ lMiddle frontal gyrus 24.25" 16.50” 40.75" Inferior frontal gyrus 1525* 16.00" 31.25” Broca’s area 4.38“ 3.75* 8.13* Premotor area 9.88“ 9.88" 19.75“ Precentral gyrus 6.13* 1 1.88** 18.00“ Paracentral gyrus 5.25“ 5.63* 10.88** Cingulate gyrus 17.88* 15.75" 33.63" Postcentral gyrus 9.13* 16.50“ 25.63* Supramarginal gyrus 26.25 ** 19.50“ 45.75" Angular gyrus 11.38" 21 .50* 32.88" Superior parietal Iobule 17.38" 30.25" 47.63” Precuneus 10.50* 13.50* 2400* Superior Temporal gyrus: Anterior 1.63 0.50 2.13 Superior Temporal gyrus: Posterior 363* 4.38 800* Middle Temporal gyrus: Anterior 0.38 0.75 1.13 Middle Temporal gyrus: Posterior 6.63" 6.88* 13.50“ Inferior Temporal gyrus: Anterior 1.38 0.38 1.75 Inferior Temporal gyrus: Posterior 5.13* 7.38" 12.50" Insula 10.88" 14.25* 25.13" Total 217.00 248.25 465.25 Table 2. Average activation of brain regions in the l-R subtraction condition. ** activated in all subjects * activated in a majority of the subjects In this condition Broca’s area is activated in the majority of subjects both in the right and left hemispheres. The posterior superior temporal gyrus is also activated in the left hemisphere for most subjects. 3.1.3 R-l Contrast In the R-l condition, the overall activation is much lower than in either of the previous contrasts, making identification of areas activated across subjects more difficult. Activation in many anatomical area were close to zero, so it is possible that variation in extent of activation was below threshold in some 41 subjects. Nonetheless several areas were still consistently activated. These included the left middle frontal gyrus, the left precentral gyrus, and the left supramarginal gyrus. The left posterior superior temporal gyrus showed activation in a majority of subjects. Notably absent is consistent activation in Broca’s area. Region Left Right Total Superior frontal gyrus 4.00“ 11.63“ 15.63“ Middle frontal gyrus 4.50““ 7.25“ 11.75““ Inferior frontal gyrus 3.50“ 4.13 7.63“ Broca's area 2.00 1.00 3.00“ Premotor area 3.63“ 0.63 4.25“ Precentral gyrus 11.50““ 1.63“ 13.13“ Paracentral gyrus 2.00 0.75 2.75“ Cingulate gyrus 6.50“ 3.00 9.50“ Postcentral gyrus 7.00“ 4.75“ 1 1 .75* Supramarginal gyrus 10.13““ 6.50“ 16.63““ Angular gyrus 4.88“ 2.13 7.00“ Superior parietal Iobule 3.88 6.75“ 10.63“ Precuneus 1.00 1.50 2.50 Superior Temporal gyrus: Anterior 0.25 0.00 0.25 Superior Temporal gyrus: Posterior 1.88“ 0.63“ 2.50“ Middle Temporal gyrus: Anterior 0.00 0.00 0.00 Middle Temporal gyrus: Posterior 1.00 0.00 1.00 Inferior Temporal gyrus: Anterior 0.00 0.88 0.88 Inferior Temporal gyrus: Posterior 1.25 0.63 1.88 Insula 4.00“ 4.75“ 8.75“ Total 72.88 58.50 131.38 Table 3. Average activation of brain regions in the R-l subtraction condition. *“ activated in all subjects * activated in a majority of the subjects 42 Irregular Regular Figure 1. Left and right hemisphere contrasts between regular and irregular activation in a single subject. Sidebar indicates the level of correlation of activity with the reference function. Because Broca’s area and the posterior superior temporal gyrus have been implicated for activation in grammatical processing in both aphasia and imaging studies, an ANOVA was performed for each area, but both were shown to have no significant effects by subtraction contrast. 3.2 Regularity An omnibus ANOVA was performed on the voxel activation data (before subtraction) considering three variables as possible sources of variation: 43 hemisphere (left or right), region (anterior or posterior), and regularity (regular or irregular). Anterior regions were those in the frontal lobs; posterior regions included those in the parietal lobe and in the temporal lobe, posterior to the center of the ventricles. A main effect for regularity was found, with more voxels activated for irregulars than for regulars, F (1, 62) = 25.29, p < 0.001 (see Figure 2). Regularity did not interact with any other variables. Irregular Regular Subject B Subject A Figure 2. Regular and irregular paradigms in the left hemisphere of two subjects. Sidebar indicates the level of correlation of activity with the reference function. 3.3 Regional Analysis In order to identify the particular regions that exhibit the greatest distinction between regulars and irregulars, eight major regions were identified in each hemisphere, based on general functional similarities and distinctions and, particular to the present study, commonalities and distinctions in patterns of activation. Anterior regions included Broca’s area, the precentral gyrus, the anterior cingulate, and the remaining portions of the prefrontal cortex (i.e., not including Broca’s area). Posterior regions included the postcentral gyrus, the insula, the posterior temporal lobe (treated as one unit), and the areas of the supramarginal gyrus, the angular gyrus, and the superior parietal Iobule combined as one unit (SMG/AG/SPL). Regions showing a significant effect for regularity were the right precentral gyrus (F (1,14) = 4.66, p < 0.05), the left prefrontal cortex (F (1,14) = 9.66, p < 0.01), the right SMG/AG/SPL (F (1,14) = 8.81, p < 0.02), the right posterior temporal lobes (F (1,14) = 8.89, p < 0.01, and the left insula (F (1,14) = 4.74, p < 0.05). In each of these cases irregular activation was significantly more widespread than regular. 3.4 Lateralization The ANOVA also revealed a trend toward a greater activation in the left hemisphere than in the right, F (1, 62) = 2.72, p = 0.10 (see Figure 1). However, when the regular and irregular contrasts were examined in relation to the eight major regions described above, it was found that the regular condition showed a significant effect for hemisphere (F (1,126) = 4.37, p < 0.05) while the irregular did not (F (1,126) = 1.15 p = 0.29). 45 4 Discussion The goal of the study was to determine whether fMRI analysis of participle and plural formation in German could shed light on the nature of the mechanism for inflection. Both theoretical paradigms are able in many cases to account for the same set of facts — it is for this reason that discussion still continues as to which model best represents the human language system. Proponents of each theory can in cases use the same data to draw different conclusions. However, the results of this study show distinctly different patterns of activation for regular and irregular German inflection which is most consistent with the dual-route theory of inflection. 4.1 Levels of activation The distinction in regular and irregular patterns is seen in several areas of the study. First, irregulars are associated with significantly higher levels of activation than are regulars. This is a well attested pattern, and seems to point to a higher processing load involved in irregulars (e.g. Jaeger et al. 1996, lndefrey et al. 1997). This also corresponds to higher RTs often observed for low- frequency irregulars (e.g. Seidenberg 1992). Jaeger et al. in fact demonstrate a correlation between long RTs and extent of neural activation. Here there is a broad distinction between regular and irregular activation where single-route connectionist arguments predict none. On a dual process view, this could be interpreted as showing higher processing costs for memory retrieval tasks than for symbolic manipulation which is blind to the specifics of individual tokens or their frequencies. Operating on variables (verb and noun stems), the regular rule can be seen as less demanding of neural resources than 46 associative processes which are sensitive both to frequency and token specificity. A connectionist model for English might explain the difficulty of irregulars in terms of frequency effects, as more common regular endings would naturally require less processing. In German, however, the predominance of irregular forms makes such an argument impossible. 4.2 Hemispheric differences These data indicates a significant difference between regular and irregular verbs by hemisphere. Areas activated by regulars predictably show more left hemisphere activation than right, as right-handed individuals are predominantly left hemisphere dominant for language. However, areas activated during irregular inflection are distributed bilaterally (no significant difference by hemisphere. Figure 1 in section 3.1.3 illustrates these hemispheric differences in activation. If one assumes, following a dual-route account, that the regular forms utilize a linguistic, rule-based process and that irregulars do not, then it is reasonable that they would show a the characteristic linguistic left hemisphere dominance. Purely memory based processes, like the processing of irregular words, are associated with no such hemisphere preference. This differentiation in lateralization is inconsistent with a single-route account but follows naturally from a dual-route account. 4.3 Regions of activation The pattern of activation common to both regular and irregular forms shows some agreement with previous studies. In table 4 the localization of 47 activation in the current study is compared with that of three earlier neuroimaging studies. Jaeger et al. lndefrey et al. Ullman et al. Current study 1996 1997 1997 Left R ht Left R ht Bilateral Left R' ht refrontal cortex u frontal iddle frontal nferior frontal ’5 area remotor area recentral s tral ulate ma inal ular u tal Iobule nferior Iobule RGUS u tem iddle tern nferior tern l R nsula I Table 4. Areas of activation for regulars and irregulars in previous neuroimaging studies compared with results from the current study. I: Activation for irregulars R: Activation for regulars B: Activation for both Previous studies show mixed findings for activation in Broca’s area. Jaeger et al. (1996) find activation for both regular and irregular verbs in Broca’s area, while Rhee et al. (1999) only see activation in Broca’s area for regulars. This data shows the unexpected effect of greater and more consistent activation for irregulars in Broca’s area than for regulars. Because Broca’s area is generally associated with grammatical rules (e.g. Grodzinsky 2000), a dual process account would predict activation of this area particularly for regulars, while the opposite is observed. Neither would a single-route account explain this 48 finding: according to such an account the activity involved in both regular and irregular processing should be the same. This confusing finding may be explained in part by the low level of activation in the regular paradigms compounded by cluster thresholding after subtraction may have increased type I error for some regular activation. It is also possible that the variation in definition of Broca’s area may account for some of the different findings. The definition of Broca’s area used for localization of regional activation in this study may have been more conservative than the definition in other studies. The left superior temporal gyrus is activated in the common map, corroborating findings of Pinker et al. (1999) that both regular and irregular inflection activated this area. Jaeger et al. (1996) points out that a major area of activation known to be involved in linguistic memory traces is the middle temporal gyrus. In the present study, as in that of Jaeger et al., this area is found to be activated consistently in the l-R contrast but not in the R-I contrast. This is consistent with a dual-route theory which associates memory primarily with irregular verbs. The middle temporal gyrus is also activated by common areas, which is predicted by both theories because all indicate that some memory must be accessed during inflection. Thus the middle temporal activation in the common area could be accounted for in a dual-route theory by necessary accessing of the lexicon for the uninflected form, and the activation specific to irregulars to the accessing of the inflected form. This latter activation would be 49 absent in regular inflection because the inflected form is produced by a rule and not located in the lexicon. With regards to the larger major regions discussed in 3.3, it is noteworthy that every region which showed significant differences between the extent of regular and irregular activation had significantly greater activation for irregulars than regulars. This is compatible with the finding that there is more extensive irregular activation in the brain overall. While it is not clear why particular regions show more distinction between regular and irregular than others, the fact of such a distinction is still clearly more compatible with a dual-route than with a single- route account. 50 5 Conclusion The findings reported in the present study are clearly preliminary. Further analysis will determine if differences exist between regular and irregular nouns and verbs and, at a finer level of analysis, between different classes of irregular verbs. These analyses may yield a more precise understanding of the nature of inflection. Nonetheless, even these preliminary findings tell us in gross terms that the brain behaves differently for regulars than for irregulars. To summarize: (1) irregulars show greater activation than regulars both overall and, where significant differences were found, within each of the eight major regions investigated, (2) regulars show greater lateralization to the left hemisphere than do irregulars, and (3) brain regions differentially respond to this regular-irregular distinction, with some regions showing more distinction than others. These broad findings as most consistent with a dual-route, symbol-manipulating account of Inflection. 51 REFERENCES Baayen, H., Piepenbrock, R., & van Rijn, H. 1995. The CELEX lexical database (CD-ROM). Linguistic Data Consortium, University of Pennsylvania. Cao, Yue, V. L. Towle, D. N. Levin, and J. M. Balter. 1993. Functional mapping of human motor cortical activation by conventional MRI at 1.5 T. Journal of Magnetic Resonance Imaging 3.869-875. Chomsky, Noam. 1986. Knowledge of Language. NY: Praeger. Clahsen, Harald. 1993. lnflectional rules in children’s grammars: evidence from the development of participles in German. Yearbook of Morphology 1992, ed. by G. Booij and J. van Marie, 1-34. Dordrecht: Kluwer. Clahsen, Harald. 1997. The representation of participles in the German mental lexicon: evidence for the dual-mechanism model. Yearbook of Morphology 1996.73-95. Clahsen, Harald. 1999. Lexical entries and rules of language: a multidisciplinary study of German inflection. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22.991-1060. Clahsen, Harald, S. Eisenbeiss, and Martina Penke. 1996. Lexical learning in early syntactic development. Generative perspectives on language acquisition, ed. by Clahsen, Harald. Benjamins. Clahsen, Harald, S. Eisenbeiss. and l. Sonnenstuhl-Henning. 1997. Morphological structure and the processing of inflected words. Theoretical Linguistics 23.201-249. Clahsen, Harald and Monika Rothweiler. 1993. lnflectional rules in children’s grammars: evidence from the development of participles in German. Yearbook of Morphology 199221-34. Clahsen, Harald, Monika Rothweiler, Andreas Woest, and Gary F. Marcus. 1992. Regular and irregular inflection in the acquisition of German noun plurals. Cognition 45.225-255. Damasio, H. 1995. Human brain anatomy in computerized images. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Grodzinsky, Y. 2000. The neurology of syntax: Language use without Broca's area. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 231-71. 52 Hagiwara, Hiroko, Yoko Sugioka, Takane Ito, Mitsuru Kawamura, and Jun-lchi Shiota. 1999. Neurolinguistic evidence for rule-based nominal suffixation. Language 75.739-763. Hagoort, Peter, and Peter lndefrey, Colin Brown, Hans Herzog, Helmuth Steinmetz, and Rildiger J. Seitz. 1999. The neural circuitry involved in the reading of German words and pseudowords: a PET study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 11.383-398. Hare, M., Elman, J., & Daugherty, K. 1995. Default generalization in connectionist networks. Language and Cognitive Processes.10, 601-630. Henderson, E. J. A. 1965. The domain of phonetics (inagural lecture). London. lndefrey, P., Colin Brown, Peter Hagoort, Hans Herzog, M. Sach, and Rtidiger J. Seitz. 1997. A PET study of cerebral activation patterns induced by verb inflection. Neurolmage 5.8548. Jaeger, Jeri J., Alan H. Lockwood, David L. Kemmerer, Robert D. Van Valin, Jr., Brian W. Murphy, and Hanif G. Khalar. 1996. A positron emission tomographic study of regular and irregular verb morphology in English. Language 72.451-497. MacWhinney, Brian and Jared Leinbach. 1991. Implementations are not conceptualizations: revising the verb learning model. Cognition 40.121-157. Marcus, Gary P, Steven Pinker, Michael T. Ullman, M. Hollander, T.J. Rosen, and F. Xu. 1992. Overregularization in language acquisition. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 57 (4, Serial No. 228). Marcus, Gary F., Ursula Brinkmann, Harald Clahsen, Richard Wiese, and Steven Pinker. 1995. German inflection: the exception that proves the rule. Cognitive Psychology 29.189-256. Marcus, Gary F. 1998. Rethinking eliminative connectionism. Cognitive Psychology 37.243-282. Marcus, Gary F. 1998. The Algebraic Mind: Integrating Connectionism and Cognitive Science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Marslen-Wilson, William D., and Lorraine K. Tyler. 1997. Nature 387.592-594. McClelland, James L., and Mark S. Seidenberg. 2000. Why do kids say goed and brang? Science 287.47-48. Mante, Thomas F., Tessa Say, Harald Clahsen, Kolja Schiltz, and Marta Kutas. 1999. Decomposition of morphologically complex words in English: evidence from event-related brain potentials. Cognitive Brain Research 7.241-253. 53 Oldfield, RC. 1971. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh Inventory. Neuropsychologia 9.97-113. Penke, Martina, Helga Weyerts, Matthias Gross, Elke Zander, Thomas F. MUnte, Harald Clahsen. 1997. How the brain processes complex words: an event- related potential study of German verb inflections. 1997. Cognitive Brain Research 6.37-52. Penke, Martina, Ulrike Janssen, and Marion Krause. 1999. The representation of inflectional morphology: evidence from Broca’s aphasia. Brain and Language 68.225-232. Pinker, Steven. 1991. Rules of language. Science 253.530-535. Pinker, Steven. 1997. Words and rules in the human brain. Nature 387.547- 548. Pinker, Steven. 1999. Words and rules: the ingredients of language. New York: Basic Books. Pinker, Steven, and Alan Prince. 1988. On language and connectionism: analysis of a parallel distributed processing model of language acquisition. Cognition 28.73-193. Plaut, David C., James L. McClelland, Mark S. Seidenberg, and Karalyn E. Patterson. 1996. Understanding normal and impaired word reading: computational principles in quasi-regular domains. Psychological Review 103.56-115. Plunkett, Kim, and Patrick Juola. 1999. A connectionist model of English past tense and plural morphology. Cognitive Science 23.463-490. Plunkett, Kim, and V. Marchman. 1991. U-shaped learning and frequency effects in a multi-layered perceptron: implications for child language acquisition. Cognition 38.43-102 Plunkett, Kim, and V. Marchman. 1993. From rote learning to system building: acquiring verb morphology in children and connectionist nets. Cognition 48.21-69. Prasada, Sandeep, Steven Pinker, and William Snyder. 1990. Some evidence that irregular forms are retrieved from memory but regular forms are rule- generated. Poster presented at the 31St Annual Meeting of the Psychonomic Society, New Orleans. 54 Pugh, Kenneth R., Bennet A. Shaywitz, Sally E. Shaywitz, Donald P. Shankweiler, Leonard Katz, Jack M. Fletcher, Pawel Skudlarski, Robert K. Fulbright, R. Todd Constable, Richard A. Bronen, Cheryl Lacadie, and John C. Gore. 1997. Predicting reading performance from neuroimaging profiles: the cerebral basis of phonological effects in printed word identification. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 23.299-318. Rhee, Jaemin, Steven Pinker, and Michael T. Ullman. 1999. A magnetoencephalographic study of English past tense production. Paper presented at the sixth annual meeting of the Cognitive Neuroscience Society, Washington, DC. Rumelhart, David E., and James L. McClelland. 1986. On learning the past tense of English verbs. Parallel distributed processing: explorations in the microstructure of cognition, ed. by James L. McClelland, David E. Rumelhart, and the PDP research group. Volume 2: Psychological and biological models, 216-271. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Say, Tessa and Harald Clahsen. In Press. Words, rules and stems in the Italian mental lexicon. In S. Nooteboom, F. Weerman, & F.Wijnen (Eds). Storage and computation in the language faculty. Amsterdam: Kluwer. Seidenberg, Mark S. 1992. Connectionism without tears. Connectionism: theory and practice, ed. by Steven Davis, 84—122. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Seidenberg, Mark S. 1997. Language acquisition and use: learning and applying probabilistic constraints. Science 275.1599-1603. Seidenberg, Mark S. and James H. Hoeffner. 1997. Evaluating behavioral and neuroimaging data on past tense processing. Language 74.104-122. Ullman, Michael T. 1999. Evidence that tool naming and grammatical rule use are subserved by frontal/basal-ganglia circuits. Paper presented at the sixth annual meeting of the Cognitive Neuroscience Society, Washington, DC. Ullman, Michael T., Ruth Bergida, and Kathleen M. O’Craven. 1997. Distinct fMRI activation patterns for regular and irregular past tense. Neurolmage 5.S549. Ullman, Michael T., Suzanne Corkin, Marie Coppola, Gregory Hickok, John H. _ Growdon, Walter J. Koroshetz, and Steven Pinker. 1997. A Neural dissociation within language: evidence that the mental dictionary is part of declarative memory, and that grammatical rules are processed by the procedural system. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 9.226-276. 55 Westermann, G., & Goebel, R. 1995. Connectionist rules of language. Proceedings of the seventeenth annual conference of the Cognitive Science Society, ed. by JD. Moore & J.F. Lehman, 236-241. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Weyerts, Helga, and Harald Clahsen. 1994. Netzwerke und symbolische Regeln im Spracheniverb: Experimentelle Ergebnisse zur Entwicklung der Flexionsmorphologie. Linguistiche Berichte 154.430-460. Weyerts, Helga, Thomas F. Milnte, Henderikus G. O. M. Smid, and Hans-Jochen Heinze. 1996. Mental representations of morphologically complex words: an event-related potential study with adult humans. Neuroscience Letters 206.125-128. Weyerts, Helga. 1997. Regulare und irregulare Flexion: Psycholinguisische und neurophysiologische Ergebnisse zu Eniverb, Verarbeitung und mentaler Representation. Dissertation. University of Dilsseldorf. Weyerts, Helga, Martina Penke, Ulrike Dohrn, Harald Clahsen, and Thomas F. MUnte. 1997. Brain potentials indicate differences between regular and irregular German plurals. NeuroReport 8.957-962. Wunderlich, Dieter, and Ray Fabri. 1995. Minimalist morphology: an approach to inflection. Zeitschrift filr Sprachwissenschaft 14.236-294. 56 1. Verbs Ten Practice Items: APPENDIX Two dummy items to get subjects started: Infinitive Participle Jaresented produced sehen gesehen stellen gestellt gehen gegangen schliessen geschlossen tragen getragen brennen gebrannt weisen gewiesen machen gemacht ziehen gezogen kommen gekommen Infinitive Participle presented produced geben gegeben fragen gefragt 57 Test items presented randomly Participle Infinitive Participle frequenc word type presented produced y 1lrregrlarABA braten gebraten 7 lrregularABB kriechen gekrochen 6 Regular knallen geknallt 7 2lrregular ABA fressen gefressen 27 lrregularABB fliessen geflossen 26 Regular tilgen getilgt 27 3IrregularABA blasen geblasen 28 lrregularABB frieren gefroren 28 Regular pflanzen gepflanzt 28 4IrregularABA heissen geheissen 33 lrregularABB fechten gefochten 32 Regular rauben geraubt 33 5lrregularABA graben gegraben 35 lrregularABB weichen gewichen 39 Regular hoffen gehofft 35 6lrregularABA essen gegessen 48 lrregularABB schreiten geschritten 44 Regular Iagern gelagert 49 7lrregular ABA waschen gewaschen 48 lrregularABB leiden gelitten 47 Regular schleppen geschleppt 51 8lrregular ABA schlafen geschlafen 79 lrregularABB streichen gestrichen 78 Regular reisen gereist 79 9Irregular ABA stossen gestossen 171 lrregularABB schiessen geschossen 170 Regular handeln gehandelt 172 10IrregularABA Iaufen gelaufen 216 lrregularABB treiben getrieben 186 Regular zahlen gezahlt 208 11lrregularABA fangen gefangen 221 lrregularABB greifen gegriffen 211 Regular planen geplant 223 12lrregularABA messen gemessen 235 lrregularABB stehen gestanden 254 Regular holen geholt 238 58 2. Nouns Ten Practice Items: Singular Plural presented produced Kind Kinder Komitee Komitees Wagen Wéigen Tisch Tische Bild Bilder Stuhl Stiihle Ei Eier Krankheit Krankheiten Bein Beine Arbeiter Arbeiter Two dummy items to get subjects started: Singular Plural presented produced Tasse Tassen Restaurant Restaurants 59 Test items presented randomly plural Singular Plural frequenc word type presented produced y 1lrregular Flirstentum Ftirstentiimer 2 Regular Bikini Bikinis 2 2lrregular Biest Biester 2 Regular Piano Pianos 2 3lrregular Viech Viecher 3 Regular Kobra Kobras 3 4Irregular Maul Mauler 5 Regular Krimi Krimis 6 5lrregular Lamm Lammer 6 Regular Oma Omas 6 6Irregular Altertum AltertiJmer 9 Regular Moskito Moskitos 9 7lrregular Leib Leiber 10 Regular Trend Trends 10 8lrregular Nest Nester 10 Regular Hobby Hobbys 10 9lrregular Gemach Gemacher 10 Regular Deck Decks 10 10lrregular Mund Miinder 11 Regular Cafe Cafes 11 11lrregular Gespenst Gespsenster 16 Regular Flamingo Flamingos 13 12lrregular Lid Lider 20 Regular Karton Kartons 19 6O IIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIII lllljlllzlljjlliljjljjljljjjllll