A . r5! {’5'}... .f. .: 74. 117.13.! C [ULRJRHH . jfi an". 11:29.53 15...! ILA.» . \ If" u: “1mm / (. J K‘ n 9- 9-02 This is to certify that the dissertation entitled Psychological Determinants of the Intention to Support Watershed Best Management Practices presented by STEPHEN R. PENNINGTON has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for Doctoral Resource Development degree in /7 Maior professor 6-20-02 Date MSU it an Affirmative Action/Equal Opportunity Institution 0- 12771 .UBRARY Michigan State University PLACE IN RETURN Box to remove this checkout from your record. To AVOID FINES return on or before date due. MAY BE RECALLED with earlier due date if requested. DATE DUE 'D’ME DUE DATE DUE SEEZ'i 2005;7- 5:1}? 23:4 I 44 DEC 1 4 2905 3 d Away 1005 £53 2 52008 93,933,091 6/01 c:/CIRCIDaleDue.p65-p.15 PSYCHOLOGICAL DETERMINANTS OF THE INTENTION TO SUPPORT WATERSHED BEST MANAGEMENT PRACTICES By Stephen R. Pennington A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Resource Development 2002 ABSTRACT PSYCHOLOGICAL DETERMINANTS OF THE INTENTION TO SUPPORT WATERSHED BEST MANAGEMENT PRACTICES By Stephen R. Pennington There is a major effort by the United States Environmental Protection Agency to convince municipalities to manage their water resources on a watershed basis. Unfortunately, there is little information as to how citizens think about managing water resources. This study uses two psychological instruments, the fairness heuristic and the theory of planned behavior in order to better understand the watershed best management practices (BMPs) peOple intend to support. Using a mail survey (n = 608) of property owners there were two key attitudes found to be held by the sample population, one based on the rights of the environment and the other reflecting people’s resistance to change. It was found that these attitudes are mediated by people’s fairness evaluations of the process in which policy is developed. Cluster analysis based on the two attitudes showed respondents held two different worldviews: individualist and egalitarian. Determinants of the intention to support BMP implementation for the individualist cluster were attitudes and locus of control whereas for the egalitarian cluster it also included personal responsibility and level of education. Knowledge was not a determinant for either cluster. Findings suggest that worldviews are a stronger predictor of the intention to support BMP implementation than knowledge. Copyright by Stephen R. Pennington 2002 I dedicate this dissertation to my wife, Kirsten. Not only did she provide support and encouragement throughout, but together with our newly born daughter, Emilee, they have taught me the true meaning of life. iv ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS Sincere appreciation is extended to my Ph.D. advisor, Dr. Scott Witter, for his support and advice. He was instrumental in the success of my Ph.D. program. His administrative skills, thoughtfulness and commitment to his students are examples I will try to follow. I am truly grateful to Dr. Jo Ann Beckwith, for her ongoing help, support and fiiendship. Her knowledge of the subject matter helped shape and strengthen this dissertation. I wish to express my gratitude to Dr. Ralph Levine, for his continuing help in the completion of this manuscript. His command of statistics and research methods not only challenged me, but also helped elevate this manuscript’s quality to the highest possible level. Special thanks are extended to Dr. Frank Fear, for his ongoing support and encouragement from the moment I arrived at Michigan State University. Beginning early on, I came to rely on his feedback to help focus my writing. His understanding of the research process helped me sort out the wheat from the chaff and produce a product I can be proud of. My sincerest appreciation to my fi’iend and Master’s thesis advisor, Dr. John FitzGibbon, for his encouragement from the very start. Without his suggestion to continue my education, I wouldn’t be where I am today. I want to thank and acknowledge both the Department of Environmental Quality and Michigan State University Extension for their financial support of my research. The financial support provided by each helped me stay on track and finish. For this my wife and l are truly grateful. I was fortunate to have two peOple proof reading this manuscript. My wife, whose attention to detail saved me on more than one occasion. I am not only deeply gratefiil to her for her love, support and encouragement but for her keen mind. My other proofreader was my sister, Dr. Lori Pennington-Gray. The countless hours of discussion, her pertinent insight, not to mention love and support were instrumental in getting me over the inevitable hurdles one encounters along the way. To my family, I express my sincerest gratitude for their understanding, patience and encouragement throughout my education. Special thanks to Mom and Dad for making the return to university a possibility back in 1993 and to my mother in-law, Linda Ackley for the countless hours of babysitting that allowed me to complete the dissertation. vi TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES IX LIST OF FIGURES XI CHAPTER 1: INTRODUCTION AND STATEMENT OF PROBLEM - _ - 1 Theoretical Perspective .................................................................................................. 9 Purpose Statement ........................................................................................................ 13 Research Questions ...................................................................................................... l3 Delimitations ................................................................................................................ 15 Definitions ................................................................................................................... 15 Organization of Study ................................................................ - .................................. 17 CHAPTER II: REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE 18 PART I 19 Values .......................................................................................................................... 19 The Role of Values in Public Decision-Making ........................................................... 20 Value Orientations ....................................................................................................... 27 Value Orientations and the Social Construction of Worldviews .................................. 30 The Evolution of the Fairness Heuristic as an Attitudinal Measure ............................. 36 Basic Categories of Environmental Policy and Implied Justice Concerns ................... 41 PART II 45 The Theories of Reasoned Action (TRA) and Planned Behavior (TPB) ...................... 45 The Behavioral Intention - Behavior Relationship ....................................................... 48 Attitudes ....................................................................................................................... 48 Personal Responsibility ................................................................................................ 53 Locus of Control (LOC) ............................................................................................... 55 Knowledge ................................................................................................................... 57 Demographic Variables ................................................................................................ 61 Predicting and Explaining Intentions and Behavior Using TRA & TPB ..................... 62 Summary ...................................................................................................................... 63 CHAPTER III: METHODOLOGY - - - 66 Study Design ................................................................................................................ 66 Background .................................................................................................................. 66 Population and Sample ................................................................................................ 67 Instrumentation ............................................................................................................ 69 Data Analysis ............................................................................................................... 76 vii CHAPTER IV: DATA ANALYSIS AND INTERPRETATION - - 79 Sample Profile and Biases ............................................................................................ 79 Demographic Characteristics ....................................................................................... 80 Constructing the Attitudinal Clusters ........................................................................... 84 Cluster Demographics .................................................................................................. 90 Characteristics .............................................................................................................. 91 Fairness Evaluations .................................................................................................... 93 Comparison Between Individualist and Egalitarian Clusters’ Fairness Evaluations 98 Relationships Between Knowledge, Locus of Control and Personal Responsibility and the Individualist and Egalitarian Clusters .................................... 101 The Individualist and Egalitarian Clusters and the Intention to Act ........................... 104 Correlations Between Study Variables ....................................................................... 106 Variables Having the Greatest Influence on the Intention to Act ............................... 110 Non-Response Results ............................................................................................... 113 Study Limitations ....................................................................................................... 114 CHAPTER V: CONCLUSIONS, DISCUSSION AND RECOMMENDATIONS. 116 Conclusions and Discussion ....................................................................................... 116 Implications for the Management of Sycamore Creek Watershed ............................. 132 Recommendations for Future Research ..................................................................... 134 APPENDICIES - _ -- -- 136 Appendix A: Survey And Response Rate Data .......................................................... 137 Appendix B: Response Frequencies For Survey Questions ....................................... 154 Appendix C: Exploratory Factor Analysis Output ..................................................... 173 Appendix D: Cluster Analysis Output ....................................................................... 184 Appendix E: Fairness Heuristic Analysis Output ....................................................... 187 Appendix F: BMP Analysis .................................................................... 208 Appendix G: Regression Analysis Output ................................................................. 210 BIBLIOGRAPHY _ _ -_ _ - _ 217 viii Table I: Table 2: Table 3: Table 4: Table 5: Table 6: Table 7: Table 8: Table 9: Table 10: Table 11: Table 12: Table 13: Table 14: Table 15: Table 16: Table 17: Table 18: LIST OF TABLES Grid/Group Nomenclature and Worldviews .................................................. 35 Summary of Findings from Meta-Analyses of the TRA and TPB ................. 62 Examples of How Different Effect Size Can Give a Different Impression 63 Summary of Findings on the Strength of the Relationships Between Psychological Variables ................................................................................ 65 Research Questions, Study Variables and the Items on the Questionnaire. . . .77 Demographic Characteristics of Respondents Compared to the Population of Ingham County .......................................................................................... 80 Principle Component Factor Analysis Results of 20 Heuristic Statements 85 Correlation Matrix of the Three Factors ......................................................... 86 Attitudinal Factor Items ................................................................ 87 Means of Fairness Heuristic Factors by Clusters ........................................... 89 Demographic Profiles of Cluster Groups ....................................................... 91 Percentage Distribution, Mean Score and Standard Deviation of Responses to Fairness Heuristic Statements .................................................. 94 Percentage of Respondents Who Agreed with and Ranked Preference for Each Fairness Heuristic Statement .................................................. 96 Comparison of Study Variables by Attitudinal Groups ................................ 102 Percentage of Respondents Who Support and Ranked Preference for Each of the Best Management Practice Options .......................................... 105 Pearson Correlations (N = 608) Between Study Variables .......................... 107 Partial Correlations Coefficients .................................................................. 108 Multiple Regression Estimates for the Intention to Support BMP Implementation ............................................................................................ l 1 1 ix Table 19: By Cluster Regression Estimates for the Intention to Support BMP Implementation ............................................................................................ 1 l3 Figure 1: Figure 2: Figure 3: Figure 4: Figure 5: Figure 6: LIST OF FIGURES Proposed Model of Responsible Environmental Behavioral Intentions Based on Hines, Hungerford and Tomera (1986) The Cognitive Hierarchy Path Models for Theory of Reasoned Action (A) and Theory of Planned Behavior (B) Map of Sycamore Creek Watershed in Relation to Ingham County, Michigan Hines, Hungerford and Tomera’ s (1986) Proposed Model of Responsible Environmental Behavior Cluster Membership and Centers by Factors. ............... xi 14, 71 20 -46 67 -- 70 “-89 CHAPTER I: INTRODUCTION AND STATEMENT OF PROBLEM Twenty-five years of water pollution regulation under the 1972 Clean Water Act (CWA) has failed to achieve its stated goal to "restore and maintain the chemical, physical, and biological integrity of the Nation's waters" [CWA, 1972] largely because of the Environmental Protection Agency’s (EPA) inability to control nonpoint source pollution. Nonpoint source (NPS) pollution or polluted runoff remains a leading cause of water pollution in both agricultural and urban areas (USEPA, 1997) and therefore the impacts are everywhere. Regardless of the large expenditures by industry and municipal wastewater systems to reduce point source pollutions, forty percent of the United States’ waterways still do not meet the minimum federal guidelines (USEPA, 1997). The Clean Water Act addresses water quality problems in two ways. The Act requires states to identify waterbodies whose quality does not support their designated beneficial uses. Total Maximum Daily Loads (TMDLs) must then be developed for each of the listed segments based on the assimilation capacity of the system. In many cases, structural best management practices (BMPs) have to be implemented to obtain the required reduction in pollutant concentrations. In addition, Phase I of the National Pollution Discharge Elimination System’s (NPDES) stormwater program requires medium and large cities (100,000+ people) to obtain permits for the discharge of stormwater runoff. Phase II of the stormwater program targets municipalities under 100,000 pe0ple and requires them to have stormwater management plans in place by March, 2003. These permits contain requirements for implementation of BMPs mostly nonstructural, however, there is a growing awareness that additional measures such as structural BMPs, may be necessary to achieve the desired quality of storm water discharges. Many metropolitan and regional agencies also require stormwater treatment in environmentally sensitive areas. Consequently, structural BMPs designed to improve water quality are being installed at an unprecedented rate (Barrett, 2000; Roesner, Bledsoe, & Brashear, 2001). Conventional structural BMPs include extended detention basins, biofilters (vegetated strips and swales), sand filters, infiltration devices and wet basins to name a few. Nonstructural BMPs typically take the form of management guidelines, regulations and information and education (I & E) programs. Under pressure from regulators, environmentalists and other stakeholders, structural BMPs are often installed without regard to the nature of the impairment of the receiving water nor the impact to the local community (Barrett, 2000). The goal of this research is to understand how pe0ple think about their local watershed and the BMP strategies for improving water quality they would support. Water-resource scientists have presented a convincing case over the years for giving greater weight to the human dimensions of water management, but have identified a number of problems in trying to accomplish this goal (Dunlap & Scarce, 1991; Harris, 1977; Kempton, Boster, & Harley, 1995; Satterfield & Gregory, 1998). Over 30 years ago, Biawas and Durie (1971) argued that water resource decisions ought to be based primarily on social criteria because the ultimate goal is to improve the quality of human life. They suggested that planners not restrict themselves to technical and economic measures in evaluating alternatives, but seek out and apply human dimension factors as well. Since that time, many researchers, including Biawas (1973), have concluded that incorporating social dimensions will make the planning process more relevant and meaningful, but at the same time will render a complicated process even more complex (Dunlap & Scarce, 1991; Harris, 1977; Satterfield & Gregory, 1998; Syme & Nancarrow, 1997b) There has been surprisingly little research on whether there are consistent dimensions in the ways in which people integrate their thoughts about water resources. The studies available have been conducted from the perspective of a particular activity, use or amenity associated with water. For example, Syme and Williams (1993c) examined the structure of perceptions in the context of the aesthetics of drinking water. Smith and colleagues (1991, 1992) have examined color and clarity in terms of recreational use of water for bathing. Other authors have discussed the meaning of water in cultural or spiritual terms (Woolmington & Burgess, 1983). Yet, there are a few research studies that try to empirically substantiate the dimensions surrounding water resources. In the context of water planning, Harris (1977) provided the first multi- dimensional analysis of the conceptions of water in an attempt to find some underlying dimensions that could assist planners within a multi-obj ective decision-making framework. With a sample of three hundred respondents from disparate subgroups, he used multi—dimensional scaling techniques to find five principal vectors relating to (1) quality of drinking water, (2) allocation and conservation, (3) natural beauties of water, (4) public involvement and (5) public access to water bodies. These vectors were consistent between subgroups (e. g. social scientists and water engineers). Nancarrow et al. (1996-97) found similar dimensions existed in their two studies. The first study was conducted on 1080 residents of three Australian cities (Perth, Canberra and Sydney) while the second, conducted two years later, was a socio-economic stratified sample of the residents of Perth. Both used personal interview methodology for administering the questionnaire. They found three common factors: aesthetics, conservation and utility. When they tried to cluster respondents based upon these factors a four-cluster solution was obtained. They named these: "Self-Interested", "Earthy", "Environmentalists" and "Service Oriented" people. Interestingly, when the water rights statement was included, the difference between the cluster means for the three factors mentioned above was not evident. It was found that the water rights statement removed the discriminating influence between clusters. In recent years the concerns about increasing the efficiency and effectiveness of water management have meant that issues such as pr0perty rights, the rights of the environment and the social and economic bases underpinning management decisions have demanded greater attention (Syme & Nancarrow, 1997a). The basic underlying themes in most planning disputes relate to what is just, fair and equitable in terms of who should benefit from planning, who should bear the costs and how should the decisions be made (Syme & Nancarrow, 1997b). Government policies constantly state that resources will be allocated equitably, yet the area that has received the least attention is the definition of what is just, fair or equitable as seen by the range of stakeholders in watershed planning decisions (Syme & Nancarrow, l997b). Syme and Fenton (1993a) attempted to examine the structure of equity and proportional preferences (Rasinski, 1987) for allocation decision-making for groundwater in Perth, Australia. There was a partial replication of Razinski’s two-factor (equality and prOportionality) structure of equity. Furthermore, there was a stated preference for arbitration procedures in dispute settlement (Lind & Tyler, 1988). Despite these encouraging results, the qualitative feedback from respondents indicated that the two-factor notion of equity did not incorporate all the subtleties of values that people consider to be important in water allocation planning. Also, the preferences provided by the Razinski’s (1987) framework were thought by some to be too simple for decision-making with multiple conflicts. As a result of these conclusions, 3 series of studies aimed at establishing universal fairness criteria were undertaken. The second study tried to broaden the measures of the philosophical basis (Wenz, 1988) on which the community might derive fairness perceptions. Such concepts as people’s attitudes towards short and long-term planning and the concept of procedural justice were tested from a wider philosophical base (Syme, Nancarrow, & McCreddin, 1999). The 111 philosophical statements moved fi'om virtue theory (people who already have the resource are inherently deserving), through ideas of the common good and differing formulations of benefit/cost analysis. When the philosophical questions were examined thematically, there was very clear support for the following positions: (1) water is a common good and should be managed for the welfare of the community as a whole; (2) more than market mechanisms are required for an adequate holistic allocation policy; (3) efficiency of use is an important component when considering allocation; (4) there is a moral obligation that human users affect others as little as possible; (5) water quality should be considered as well as quantity; (6) there is an obligation for general public information or involvement in allocation decisions; and (7) the environment has allocation ‘rights’(Syme & Nancarrow, 1996). Interestingly, there was only a modest correlation between long-term planning (certainty) and agreement on allocation decisions being short-term and dependent on the circumstances at the time (Syme & Nancarrow, 1996). The interaction between these dimensions would seem to indicate the importance of framing fairness judgments in particular situations. This led to the adoption of universal fairness and situational fairness principles in later studies (Syme & Nancarrow, 1996). Universal fairness criteria are guiding principles used to evaluate all decisions where as situational principles arise only when the outcome is likely to impact the community or the individual. Also lending support to this dichotomy was the fact that respondents tended to support prior rights in their allocation priorities, but they did not support items measuring attitudes towards prior rights as a universal principle. Therefore, the subsequent studies made the conceptual distinction between fairness principles being applied differently at the universal and situational levels (Syme & Nancarrow, 1996). Several other studies served to refine and substantiate Syme and his colleagues (1999) findings under a variety of circumstances. For instance, the third study refined and applied the universal philosophies to actual decision-making systems and provided specific criteria by which overall fairness could be evaluated. Studies four, five and six which were a mixture of both qualitative and quantitative approaches, tested the fairness heuristic under differing political and social circumstances was well as over time. As a result, Syme et al., (1999) confidently make the following assertions with regard to water allocation planning in Australia: 0 A large portion of pe0p1e believe in the rights of the environment and its preservation for a range of uses for future generations. 0 Fair decision-making processes are of paramount importance to community acceptance of water allocation decisions. 0 Water markets alone are not considered fair or acceptable processes for allocating or re-allocating water. 0 Economic arguments are of a lesser importance to process considerations when deciding how water should be allocated or re-allocated. 0 Efficiency of use is a major determinant of the fairness of water allocation systems. The lessons learned from studying water allocation in Australia are more relevant in the United States today than ever before. This is because the Environmental Protection Agency and its designated state agencies have adopted a decentralized approach to watershed management (USEPA, 1997). Under this approach, public participation and deliberative decision-making procedures allow for strategies to emerge as a result of a process which brings to the table all relevant social, environmental and economic matters (USEPA, 1997). The traditional institutional lines become blurred with new formal and informal linkages being established depending upon the requirements of the intended actions (Burroughs, 1999). The aim is to create local learning organizations that have the ability to adapt to ongoing changes presented by their operating environment (Bawden, 1995) Clearly if the EPA approach is to succeed, the process must balance the needs of multiple users and uses in planning efforts and it should maximize acceptance of decisions through perceptions of having been treated 'fairly'. Conflicts in values have increased as utilitarian views are being replaced with a more environmental orientation (Brown & Harris, 1992; Dunlap & Van Liere, 1978; Eckersley, 1992; Stem, Dietz, Kalof, & Guagnano, 1995). Gallup data (Gallup, 1989) for example, show that 75% of the population now claims to be environmentalist. Researchers attribute this shift in value orientation to p0pulation growth (Manfrado & Zinn, 1996) and changes in the nation's demographics (Steel, List, & Shindler, 1994). For example, younger, more educated, urban dwellers tend to de-emphasize traditional uses of forests (e. g., logging, mining, grazing) and place higher values on issues such as wildlife preservation. There is little reason to assume that watershed issues would be any different. Because these trends are likely to continue into the future, it is important for managers to better understand the implication that diversely held values can have on planning initiatives. Theory predicts that more general value orientations affect attitudes regarding specific objects and/or situations and attitudes, in turn, influence behavior (Ajzen & Fishbein, 1977). As was just presented, Syme’s (l993,l996,1999) work suggests attitudes are moderated by fairness evaluations. Never the less, several recent studies stress the importance of environmentally centered values (including fairness) in guiding policy and management actions (Dunlap & Scarce, 1991; Kempton et al., 1995; Satterfield & Gregory, 1998). It has been argued by Gregory, Keeney, and von Winterfeldt (1992) that it is essential for environmental managers to listen closely to the concerns, fears, wisdom and preferences voiced by the people who might potentially be affected by their actions. Also, a substantial body of evidence supports the notion that experts and laypersons often view the world quite differently (Slovic, 1987). The real question then becomes how do we respond to these differences in both the planning process and subsequent outcomes? Although the importance of including human factors in water resources planning has been discussed at length, a surprisingly limited amount of research has emerged within the context of American watersheds. As a result, a critical need exists to define and describe the human domain surrounding planning within American watersheds. Iogically, little can be done towards incorporating human factors into the planning- decision process without a clear delineation of the applicable factors, a description of how the factors relate to each other and an assessment of the relative values with which the factors are held. Theoretical Perspective This research uses an attitudinal model to guide the exploration of the relationships between the antecedents variables for the intent to support the implementation of BMPs. Based upon Hines, Hungerford, and Tomera's (1986) hypothesized model the antecedents of ‘knowledge of environmental issues’, ‘knowledge of action strategies’, ‘locus of control’, ‘attitude’ and ‘personal responsibility’ are used to predict behavioral intentions towards proposed BMPs. This model is consistent with Azjen’s "Theory of Planned Behavior" (Ajzen, 1985), one of the most cited behavioral theories. According to his theory, intention to act has a direct effect on behavior and can be predicted by attitude, subjective norms and perceived behavior control. Individual values underlie both a person’s worldview and their attitudes. Therefore, people possessing similar attitudes within a p0pulation (i.e., they have the same general attitude towards an object) are ofien said to have the same worldview (Dake, 1991, 1992) and would likely have many shared values. Hines, Hungerford and Tomera's (1986) meta-analysis of variables related to environmental behavior can be categorized into cognitive, affect and situational factors. The cognitive factor, that is the awareness level about the object, is related to knowledge of the environment including action skills and strategies. The affect variables, or feelings and emotions associated with the objects, are generally defined by attitude, locus of control and responsibility. Situational factors such as economic and social constraints and/or pressures and opportunities to choose different actions either counter act or strengthen the cognitive and affect factors. Several researchers have suggested that an individual’s attitude towards the management of a resource is moderated by their perception of the fairness of the process as well as outcomes of the proposed decisions (Peterson, 1994; Syme & Nancarrow, 1997b; Syme etal., 1999). One research instrument designed to capture these interactions is the fairness heuristic. The heuristic provides a range of fairness criteria that may contribute to stakeholders overall fairness evaluations, making it possible to understand the elements of a community's view on the appropriate basis for decision-making. The fairness heuristic has been successfirlly used in the area of water allocation in Australia. Its mix of attitudes towards water, planning attitudes and lay philosophies make it an apprOpriate theoretical foundation on which to base research in American watersheds. The concept of procedural justice and the demonstration of its significance gained prominence in Lind and Tyler's seminal book - The Social Psychology of Procedural Justice (Lind & Tyler, 1988). Procedural justice research focuses on the characteristics of a decision-making process which make it seem ‘just’ to people vulnerable to the consequences of a decision (Rasinski, 1987). General dimensions of procedural justice 10 such as voice or the feeling that one has had the opportunity to influence the process have been demonstrated and replicated (Axelrod, 1994). The major hypothesis of procedural justice is that if procedural justice is demonstrated in a decision-making process, the outcome is more likely to be accepted (Axelrod, 1994; Lind & Tyler, 1988; Rasinski, 1987; Syme et al., 1999; Thibaut & Walker, 1975). Distributive justice as a concept relates to the evaluation of whether an outcome was just in terms of the distribution of resource between stakeholders. In this way, equity and distributive justice are closely related. The dimensions of equity seem to be the bases on which individuals assess whether or not distributive justice has been achieved. One of the purposes of this study is to better understand the groups of respondents holding different viewpoints about implementing BMPs within Sycamore Creek watershed in Ingham County, Michigan. Consequently, there is a need to provide a theoretical foundation for these groupings; several theories are available, but one based on people’s worldviews about the environment seems appropriate. The relational pattern of worldviews put forth by Karl Dake (1992) is rooted in cultural theory. He describes five patterns of interpersonal relationships surrounding the perceptions of environmental risk: hierarchical, individual, egalitarian, fatalist and autonomous. These relational forms are hypothesized to engender shared representations of how the environment is viewed (Douglas, 1985; Douglas & Wildavsky, 1982) and will be used to guide analysis. The planning literature is replete with examples of framing the perceptions of a problem differently (Messick, 1993). The framework ranges from the general to the more specific where the outcome is likely to impact either the community or the individual. These two terms have been differentiated in the literature by the terms universal fairness ll and situational fairness (Lind & Tyler, 1988; Syme & Nancarrow, 1996). The values that contribute to these overall judgments are labeled fairness criteria. The varied and diverse nature of BMPs and how they are implemented allow for an exploration of different policy options. If it is assumed that policy falls into one of six categories: 1) regulation, 2) taxation and other charges, 3) subsidies, 4) market mechanisms, 5) human rights and 6) voluntary, then it is conceivable to create a BMP research instrument depicting each of these. By first educating the respondent both verbally and visually about BMPs, a frame of reference will be created. The idea of flaming an approach to environmental problem solving was reviewed by Bardwell (1991). She drew together concepts from cognitive psychology and conflict management to focus upon the process of problem definition. Problem framing refers to “a concerted effort to focus on one’s understanding of a problem” (Bardwell, 1991, p. 607). The framing concepts in this study are defined the following way: 0 A frame of reference is an analytical model of values concerning a specific water resource policy or management issue. 0 A personal frame of reference refers to the values expressed by an individual. 0 A common frame of reference refers to the distinctive pattern of values common to a number of individuals. Framing the policy choices for BMP implementation was chosen in an effort to control for two of the three boundary conditions that Fishbein and Ajzen (1975; Aj zen & Fishbein, 1977) identified as being able to affect the magnitude of the relationship between attitudes and behavior. Particularly, the use of a frame of reference should increase the degree to which the measure of intention and the behavioral criterion 12 correspond with respect to their levels of specificity and it can control for the degree to which carrying out the intention is under the volitional control of the individual. Previous research indicates that the principles at the situational level are not constructed independently from those at the universal level (Ajzen & Fishbein, 1977; Axelrod, 1994). Just how universal and situational fairness criteria change and interact when people are framing their fairness judgments is not yet totally clear. However, it may be that the more urgent the situation and the greater the need for the short-term actions to achieve long-term sustainability, the more that situational fairness may dominate (Nancarrow, Smith, & Syme, 1996-7). Purpose Statement The primary purpose of the study is to assess the relationships between the antecedents of knowledge, attitudes, locus of control and sense of responsibility on the intention to support the implementation of watershed best management practices. Also, it will examine the role of individual fairness judgments towards developing watershed policies. Based on these variables, the study population will be categorized and described. Lastly, the study will determine which variables influence the intention to support the implementation of watershed best management practices. Research Questions This study addresses the following research questions: Q1: What attitudinal groups exist in the community surrounding the implementation of BMPs in the Sycamore Creek watershed? Q2: How do the demographic characteristics vary by attitudinal group? Q3: What are the “fairness evaluations” regarding the implementation of BMPs for the sample population and for each attitudinal group? 13 Q4: Q5: Q6: Q7: Q8: Q9: Q10: What is the relationship between "personal responsibility" and the attitudinal groups? What is the relationship between "knowledge of the issues" and the attitudinal groups? What is the relationship between "knowledge of action strategies" and the attitudinal groups? What is the relationship between "locus of control" and the attitudinal groups? Do the attitudinal groups differ in terms of intention to act? What are the correlations between knowledge, personal responsibility, locus of control and intent to act? Which of the variables of knowledge, attitudes, personal responsibility and locus of control have the greatest influence on one’s intention to act? (see Figure 1) Knowledge of Action Strategies Knowledge of Issues Intention to H Act Attitudes Social and Locus 0f Personality COHtI'OI Factors Personal / Responsibility Figure 1: Proposed Model of Responsible Environmental Behavioral Intentions Based on Hines, Hungerford and Tomera (1986) 14 Delimitations The study was delimited to a sample of property owners within the Sycamore Creek watershed, Ingham County, Michigan. The restriction of property ownership further delimited the study to individuals eighteen years old and above possessing the income and desire necessary to own property. Definitions The following terms are defined for the purpose of this study. For many of the terms, a more operational explanation is detailed in Chapter III. Attitude: describes the individual’s feelings, pro or con, favorable or unfavorable, with regard to particular aspects of the environment (Hines et al., 1986; Newhouse, 1990). Ajzen & Fishbein (1980) suggested that attitude categories include attitudes towards objects as well as more specific attitudes towards certain issues or attitudes toward taking action. Best management practices: can be defined as "schedules of activities, prohibitions of practices, maintenance procedures, and structural and/or managerial practices, that when used singly or in combination, prevent or reduce the release of pollutants to waters. .." (MRSC, 2000). Distributive Justice: distribution of outcomes on either an equal or equitable basis (Lind & Tyler, 1988). Equity: the value that people should receive retums appropriate to their contribution (Lind & Tyler, 1988). Equality: the value that all people should receive the same return (Lind & Tyler, 1988). 15 Intention to Act: behavioral intention is indicated by a person’s subjective perception and report of the probability that s/he will perform the behavior in question (Parcels, 1984). Intended behavior is used as a substitute for actual behavior, but may not be as an accurate predictor (Hwang, Kim, & Jeng, 2000). Knowledge: environmental knowledge can be categorized into three levels, (1) knowledge about the issues, (2) knowledge about the action strategies and (3) possessing an action skill (Boerschig & DeYoung, 1993; Hines et al., 1986). Locus of Control: is a construct that refers to an individual's beliefs about whether the outcomes of his/her actions are dependent on what his/her do (internal control orientation) or are determined by events outside his/her personal control (external control orientation) (Rotter, 1966). Personal Responsibility: means a personal obligation or sense of duty to implement actions (Boerschig & DeYoung, 1993). Procedural Justice: procedures consistent with personal values and that show dignity and respect for participants (Lind & Tyler, 1988; Tyler, 1988). Values: are standards or criteria that guide action as well as other psychological phenomena such as attitudes, judgments and attributions (Rokeach, 1979). Values are considered deeper and more stable than attitudes, representing standards of "oughts and shoulds" and are viewed as determinants of attitudes (Rokeach, 1979, p. 272). Value Orientations: clusters of interrelated fundamental values (Schwartz, 1992; Stem et al., 1995). I6 Worldviews: defined as shared beliefs and values that justify different ways of behaving with corresponding cultural biases towards different patterns of social relations (Dake, 1991,1992). Organization of Study The presentation of this research is organized into five chapters. Subsequent to this introductory chapter is a review of the literature germane to the research. The chapter is divided into two main sections. The first section reviews the theoretical underpinnings while the second focuses on specifying the relationships between the model components through a review of previous research. The following topics are discussed in the first section: values and their role in decision-making; value orientations and worldviews; and the development of the fairness heuristic as an attitudinal measure. In section II the topics discussed are: the theories of reasoned action (TRA) and planned behavior (TPB), the behavior — behavioral intention relationship, attitudes, personal responsibility, locus of control, knowledge, the influence of demographic variables and predicting and explaining intentions and behavior using TRA & TPB. The third chapter outlines the steps taken in formulating the survey instrument, how the survey was conducted and the methods used for the analysis. Chapter IV presents the attitudinal factors found in the sample population, it profiles the attitudinal groups who poses similar worldviews, it explores the fairness evaluations towards implementing BMPs, it explores the relationships between each of the study variables and it determines which variables best predict behavioral intentions. Finally, Chapter V contains the discussion and conclusions, the implications of this research to Sycamore Creek watershed managers and recommendations for further research. 17 CHAPTER II: REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE The purpose of this chapter is to review the literature on the theory of environmental decision-making and the antecedents of knowledge, attitudes, locus of control and personal responsibility thought to influence these decisions. Several researchers (Peterson, 1994; Rasinski, 1987; Syme et al., 1999) have hypothesized that fairness evaluations mediate the relationship between these antecedents and the intention to act environmentally responsible. Therefore, this chapter also reviews the literature on procedural and distributive justice in environmental decision-making and is divided into two main sections. Part I reviews the psychological theory on which the research is based and is broken down into five sections: (1) values, (2) the role of values in decision- making, (3) value orientations, (4) value orientations and the social construction of worldviews, (5) the evolution of the fairness heuristic as an attitudinal measure and (6) basic categories of environmental policy and implied justice considerations. Part II reviews previous research employing the same study variables so that a better understanding of the relationships between the model variables can be understood. Part II is divided into eight sections: (1) the theories of reasoned action (TRA) and planned behavior (TPB), (2) the behavioral intention - behavior relationship, (3) attitudes, (4) personal responsibility, (5) locus of control, (6) knowledge, (7) demographic variables and (8) predicting and explaining intentions and behavior using TRA & TPB. The chapter concludes with a summary. 18 PART I Values Although even a cursory review of the literature on human values yields a large number of definitions there are five features that are common to most of the value definitions (Maslow, 1959; Rokeach, 1973; Schwartz & Bilsky, 1987). According to the literature, values are (3) concepts or beliefs, (b) about desired end states or behaviors, (c) transcend specific situations, (d) guide selection or evaluation of behavior and events and (e) are ordered by relative importance (Schwartz, 1992). Values, as defined by Rokeach (1979), are standards or criteria that guide action as well as other psychological phenomena such as attitudes, judgments and attributions. Values are considered deeper and more stable than attitudes, representing standards of "oughts and shoulds" (Rokeach, 1979, p. 272) and are viewed as determinants of attitudes. Schwartz (1992) added "the primary content aspect of a value is the type of goal or motivational concern that it expresses" (Rokeach, 1979, p. 4). Some examples include efficiency and practicality, achievement and success, democracy, freedom, and equity, to name a few. These definitions and examples suggest that the values people embrace are responsible for guiding their pursuits in life. Theory suggests that an individual's view of the environment in which he or she lives can be organized into a hierarchy consisting of values, value orientations (i.e., patterns of basic beliefs), attitudes/norms, behavioral intentions and behaviors (Ball- Rokeach, Rokeach, & Grube, 1984; Fulton, Manfedo, & Lipscomb, 1996; Homer & Kahle, 1988; Rokeach, 197 3; Rokeach, 1979). Each of these elements build upon one another in what has been described as an inverted pyramid (Figure 2). l9 Numerous Faster to Change Peripheral Specific to Situations Behaviors Behavioral Intentions Attitudes and Norms Value Orientations (Basic Belief Patterns) Values Few in Number Slow to Change Central to Beliefs Transcend Situations Figure 2: The Cognitive Hierarchy Source: Fulton et al., 1996 Values tend to be widely shared by all members of a culture and as such are unlikely to account for much of the variability in specific attitudes and behaviors. Rather, values are reflected in attitudes via beliefs, value orientations and attitudes. For example, basic beliefs serve to strengthen and give meaning to fundamental values and visa versa. Patterns of these basic beliefs create value orientations (Fulton et al., 1996). The Role of Values in Public Decision-Making Relatively little is known about how people make political decisions under the stress of conflict. However, three possible explanations can be found in the literature to explain the apparent value conflict frequently encountered in decision-making. The first explanation gives personal values a central role in reasoning about behavioral intentions 20 (value-driven models). An example is Tetlock's (1986) value pluralism model of ideological reasoning that argues pe0ple reason in increasingly integrative complex ways about an issue to the extent it invokes values that are both highly and equally cherished. The second hypothesis is that personal values are mediated and therefore given a secondary role. The example here is Lind's (1992) fairness heuristic model that argues an assessment of ‘fairness’ mediates the relationship between values and support for policy positions. The third set of explanations is the affect hypotheses that do not allow any role for personal values in reasoning on highly emotional issues. These theories (Jackman, 1978; Schuman et al., 1985; Sears and Kinder, 1971) argue that people take positions based on affect, not cognition like values. Let us look closer at each of these theories. The Value Pluralism Approach The value pluralism approach to explaining value conflict that may result in cognitive dissonance (Festinger, 1962) is as follows: policy analysts frequently argue that efforts to achieve one objective (6. g. equality) often require sacrificing or seriously compromising other important objectives (e. g. merit). For example, policies designed to manage natural resources often have the unwanted side effect of decreasing personal income. And policies designed to increase economic growth and efficiency ofien exacerbate income inequalities. In brief, making public policy choices requires making value t1adeoffs(Sniderman, Brody, & Kuklinski, 1991; Sniderrnan & Tetlock, 1986b; Tetlock, 1986). To support a particular policy means trading fulfillment of one cherished value for another. For example, Tetlock (1986) found that when subjects ranked ‘equality’ higher than ‘a comfortable life’ as a personal value this was highly correlated (r = .61) with whether the subjects supported paying higher taxes to assist the poor. 21 Subjects believed that to achieve equality for the poor meant forfeiting personal income vis—a-vis higher taxes. Feather (1979) also found a significant correlation between value rankings and public policy choices. Using the Rokeach (1979) value survey, he found that subjects who supported politically conservative policies also supported particular values (e.g., national security, cleanliness, obedience and salvation) over others (e. g., equality, freedom, love and pleasure). Value differences therefore underlie policy choices. In an effort to explain when people will acknowledge that important values are indeed in conflict, Telock (1986) proposed a value pluralism model of ideological reasoning. The model can be summarized in the three following propositions. First, all ideologies have underlying core or terminal values (Lane, 1973; Rokeach, 1973, 1979) that direct people's public policy preferences by specifying what the goals ought to be of the public policy. Second, people differ in the degree to which they acknowledge their core values are in conflict with one another. People with monastic ideologies believe their values all point in one policy direction while people with pluralistic ideologies acknowledge their core values sometimes point in conflicting directions. And thirdly, people with more pluralistic ideologies display more integrative or "trade-ofi" reasoning than those with monistic ideologies (Peterson, 1994). In short, value pluralism is increased to the extent that core values are both more highly and equally prized, thus setting the frame for decision-making (Barnberg, Kuhnel, & Schmidt, 1999). Values are Mediated - The Fairness Heuristic Lind et al., (1995) argue that people use subjective assessments of fairness, an organizing principle that is derived in part from personal values (Rasinski, 1987), as an 22 organizing schema in social and organizational decision—making. In particular, they hypothesized the Operation of a ‘fairness heuristic’ which suggests peOple form impressions of the general fairness of an organization, authority or policy and use that as a major criterion for support or opposition to the policy (Lind, 1992). People assess fairness when encountering a new situation because they are suspicious of the intent or result of a policy. Questions like “Do programs designed to improve a situation really help the people whom they were intended to assist?” are ofien asked. Fairness here is a judgment. The judgment is defined as what "is" rather than what "ought to be" as a value judgment. In the initial assessment stage, people are highly attuned to cues about a policy's fairness including the assessment of how well a procedure shows respect and dignity for the participants (Lind & Tyler, 1988; Tyler & Lind, 1992) and if it is congruent with ones’ own personal values (Rasinski, 1987). Procedures that are consistent with personal values and show dignity and respect for participants are considered fair and are likely to be supported. Policies inconsistent with personal values and not showing dignity and respect for those involved are likely to be opposed. Once an impression of fairness is produced, it becomes extremely resistant to change because it provides a cognitively available summary judgment (Peterson, 1994). People use their summary fairness judgment in lieu of a more complicated policy analysis each time they are asked. Therefore, the fairness heuristic posits that values play a role in developing policy positions, if only a secondary one. Both correlation and experimental evidence have supported the fairness heuristic hypothesis. In a study assessing litigant reactions involving arbitration in Federal Court 23 cases, Lind et a1. (1995) found that decisions of whether to accept or reject an arbitrator's award was most strongly related to people's procedural justice judgments. Results revealed that judgments of procedural fairness mediated the effects of outcome evaluations. People who thought the process was fair were more likely to accept the mediation award, regardless of the outcome (Lind and Tyler, 1988; Tyler and Lind, 1992). Procedural justice judgments were better predictors of acceptance of a mediation award than either subjective or objective measures of the award itself (Lind and Tyler, 1988; Tyler and Lind, 1992). Finally, results showed that use of the fairness heuristic was not limited to individuals; corporate decision makers (some of the litigants represented corporations as well) similarly indicated the use of a fairness heuristic. In another field study of court-annexed arbitration hearings in New Jersey State Courts, (MacCoun, Lind, Hensler, Bryant, & Ebener, 1988) found similar results. Participants' assessments of fairness were strongly related to their perceptions of procedural fairness (i.e. neutrality of the arbitrator and a process that grants full status to those involved). Again, respondents who rated the process they experienced as more fair also were more likely to accept awards of the arbiter and were more satisfied with the final outcome of their case than those who rated their experiences as fair. Affect Explanations - No Role for Values The insincerity and minimalism theories are both affect explanations for the role of values in decision-making. These two theories posit a strikingly different role for values in people's support (or lack there of) for public policy. In particular, researchers (Jackman, 1978; Schumann, Stech, & Bobo, 1985; Sears & Kinder, 1971; Sidanius & Pratto, 1993) have noted the paradox between support for racial equality in principle and 24 opposition to government programs such as affirmative action to achieve those ends. Such ‘slippage’ between espoused values and public policy preferences is known as the “principle-policy puzzle” (Sniderrnan & Tetlock, 1986a) and is a common finding in surveys of political issues. In several arenas people's policy preferences are out of step with at least one of their expressed values. Affect theorists argue that people's espoused values are often inconsistent with policy preferences because policy preferences are dictated by deep-seated, self-centered feelings and not rational thought (Peterson, 1994). Proponents of the insincerity theory (Jackman, 1978) argue that regardless of what they say, people are not committed to the liberal values they advocate. It is argued that many Americans, particularly those well educated and most likely to purport liberal values, provide lip service to egalitarian values because those values are socially desirable when in fact, they support traditional, self-interest driven public policies. McConahay (1986) goes so far as to say that those who support traditional or conservative policies are using that as a cover for racist values. This line of argument usually reduces to an impression management explanation where peOple firmish egalitarian responses to value questions primarily to impress others. Alternatively, the minimalist argument theorize there are only loose linkages between values and policy positions which create inconsistency between values and policy positions (Converse, 1964). People have only a very hazy notion of how their values should translate into their support/opposition of policy. As a result, where strong affect is aroused, it can overwhelm the role of personal values and dominate policy preferences. Again using the example of affirmative action, many white Americans experience discomfort in the presence of Afiican Americans (Crosby, Bromley, & Saxe, 25 1980; Katz, 1976). Therefore, when reasoning about race issues if someone feels favorable towards African Americans, they favor affirmative action, but because many whites are uncomfortable around African Americans they would oppose affirmative action. The insincerity and minimalism theories have quite different implications for understanding attitudes about policy support. Belief in minimalism leads one to the conclusion that people need to be educated about the implications of the values they hold. Specifically in the case of affirmative action, it would lead in the direction of clarifying the implications of the egalitarian values to the mass public. Conversely, the insincerity hypothesis contends that education is a primary cause of the paradox of simultaneously supporting egalitarian values and opposing affirmative action. White, Anglo-Saxon public policy preferences are really driven by a desire to retain a privileged position in society (Sidanius and Pratto, 1993). Unfortunately, it is quite difficult to distinguish whether people are engaging in impression management or in intra-psychic conflict because in most situations they are empirically indistinguishable (Peterson, 1994). Empirical support for one of these theories can be interpreted as support for the other as well. The structure of human values is inherently interesting to many researchers (Bazerman, Messick, Tenbrunsel, & Wade - Benzoni, 1997; Feather, 1979; Rasinski, 1987). Schwartz and Bilsky (1987) see three main benefits to focusing on values for research purposes. First, the impacts of values as independent variables on both attitudes and behaviors can be predicted, identified and interpreted more effectively and reliably by using indices of the value orientations as opposed to single values (Ajzen & Fishbein, 26 1980; Rokeach, 1979; Schwartz & Bilsky, 1987). Second, the effects of social structural variables (i.e. economic, political, religious, ethnic) on values as dependent variables can be predicted, identified and interpreted more effectively by using value orientations as opposed to single values (Ajzen & Fishbein, 1980; Rokeach, 1979; Schwartz & Bilsky, 1987). And lastly, cross-cultural studies seem to indicate that comparisons of value importance are more comprehensive if value orientations are used because the orientations will ideally cover all the significant types of value content whose meanings are shared, where as research not guided by a concept of value structure must rely on single values arbitrarily chosen by the researcher (Ajzen & Fishbein, 1980; Rokeach, 1979; Schwartz & Bilsky, 1987). Value Orientations Much of what has been written about incorporating values into environmental management decision-making comes from the literature on economics. Contingent valuation (CV) and its variants have strived since the early 1980’s to capture how people value environmental goods and attach dollar amounts to these values. The prevailing practice is to take maximum willingness to pay (WTP) as the measure of the value of a good for an individual. This purchase model is the theoretical foundation for the CV method and the value of a public good to the public is estimated by surveys in which respondents state their willingness to pay for the good (Bazerman et al., 1997; Carson, Louviere, & Anderson, 1994). This first value orientation relies on the tenants of economic equity (Walster, Walster, & Berscheid, 1978) and utility theory (Bazerrnan et al., 1997) and refers primarily to goals such as economic security or achievement, material rewards and/or avoidance of economic, material or time costs. It parallels the 27 sustenance needs identified by Schwartz and Bilsky (1987) and Maslow (1959), although it more accurately reflects the value placed on economic and material desires regardless of one's actual need situation (Bazerman et al., 1997). This is an important distinction because the pursuit of economic gain appears to motivate behavior well beyond a time when physical needs are met (Bazerman et al., 1997). Discrepancies between peoples willingness to accept payment and willingness to pay for a public good (Bazerman et al., 1997), the high incidence of protest bids (Jorgensen & Syme, 2000) and the possibility that people view payments as contributions towards preservation (Bazerman et al., 1997) have all been cited as arguments that the ' contingent valuation method fails to capture the complexities surrounding how people value the environment. Consequently, it can be surmised that economic valuation is only one value orientation people use when making environmental decisions. Stern et al. (1993) propose two other value orientations besides economic: (a) social requirements and (b) universal. The second value orientation represents the social aspects of life. It specifies desires regarding social consequences from one's actions and includes both belongingness and conformity drivers as well as aspects of social altruism (Schwartz & Bilsky, 1987) and benevolence (Schwartz, 1992) motives. It postulates that the motivation to seek belongingness and acceptance from others is a central guiding force in decisions to act (Axelrod, 1994). Virtually all discussions on value theories have at least one domain that involves social needs (Axelrod, 1994). It is assumed that conformity and belongingness values, as well as part of the benevolent values, are rooted in relationship needs and desires. Thus actions in accordance with these desires would, theoretically, 28 lead people to pursue goals such as the welfare of others (especially those close to you) as one means of maintaining and/or enhancing one's feelings of connection with others. Social values and desires are frequently noted in discussions of environmental behavior. For instance, the desire for belongingness can induce people to act in a manner consistent with valued others and the value placed on benevolence may prompt socially- oriented people (people who place social values at the top of their hierarchy) to act environmentally protective when they believe their actions can help minimize the plight of other people (Axelrod, 1994). In addition, the social value orientation can be seen as consistent with the cooperative orientation identified by Messick and McClintock (1968), although a COOperative orientation may also overlap with the universal value domain. Numerous studies have shown that players involved in experimental game paradigms who have a cooperative orientation pursue outcomes that maximize gains for all players and not just for themselves (Kramer, McClintock, & Messick, 1986). The third motivational orientation, universal, is the most consistent with Schwartz's (1992) universalism domain. The motivational content of this value type involves the pursuit of self-respect garnered from making a contribution to the betterment of the world, especially as it pertains to pursuing and attaining outcomes that correspond with universal-type goals (e.g., equity, environmental preservation). Pursuing these goals may in fact involve certain social or economic costs, which universally-oriented individuals are willing to incur (Axelrod, 1994). For example, protesting the harvesting of a section of forest may mean a loss of jobs and have no perceived social benefits, but pe0ple may do it in response to their desire to improve environmental conditions - an outcome consistent with a desire to act in a universalistic manner. This motivational 29 domain is most strongly reflected in those people willing to violate laws and court injunctions and risk substantial fines in order to achieve a certain goal. A cautionary consideration regarding the construction of this value taxonomy should be noted. Although each domain is presumed to be an independent source of motivation, overlapping values among the domains may occur. To illustrate, universal goals may involve pursuing outcomes such as social justice - a goal that blurs the line between the social and universal orientations. This confusion can be addressed by recalling the basic motivations associated with each domain. People with a social orientation are most concerned with maintaining and enhancing connections with others. When applied to social issues, this goal is considered consistent with pursuing outcomes that are believed to benefit a majority of people. People adhering to a universal value domain embrace a contributory ethic - one which emphasizes the pursuit of a personal conception of universal goals. Value Orientations and the Social Construction of Worldviews The literature on the social construction of risk can lend insight on the nexus between value orientations and worldviews. Douglas (1975) and her colleagues argue that conflicts over risk are best understood in terms of plural social constructions of meaning. Competing cultures confer different meanings on situations, events, objects and especially relationships. Their assertion is that risk perception is everywhere and always biased by legitimized social groupings in the form of institutions embodied in everyday, ordinary social interactions with family, friends and colleagues (Douglas, 1990; Douglas, 1986; Douglas, 1985; Douglas & Wildavsky, I982a; Douglas, 1982b; Schwartz & Thompson, 1990). Indeed, this is true for more than just risk situations and the same 30 reasoning can be extended to most decision-making circumstances. Their reasoning provides a useful explanation for people’s decision-making reasoning and therefore can provide a construct for understanding them, especially as it pertains to value orientations. In making the claim that risk perceptions are socially constructed, Douglas (1975), Dake (1992) and others pr0pose a functional explanation why risk taking and risk ignoring are products of the various involvements that individuals have in their social life (Thompson, Ellis, & Wildavsky, 1990). Thus, cultural theory is a functional interpretation of the myths of nature because risk is explained in terms of the contribution a person’s perceptions have for maintaining a particular way of life (Dake, 1992). Cultural theory accounts for the social construction of the environment in terms of three linked domains that constitute a way of life: cultural biases, social relations and behavioral strategies. Worldviews are defined as shared beliefs and values that justify different ways of behaving with corresponding cultural biases towards different patterns of social relations (Dake, 1991, 1992). When environmentalists blame the system for environmental damage or when corporations proclaim a comUCOpia view of nature and call for market controls (e.g., carbon taxes) or when bureaucratic organizations call for a top-down management of technological hazards, these behaviors are functional because they justify and maintain the pattern of social relations from which they arise (Dake, 1992; Thompson et al., 1990, p.104). The social construction approach hypothesizes that identity is mediated by an individual’s relationship to others (i.e., social and universal value orientations). Individuals who identify with collectives that make decisions binding on all members "will see themselves very differently than those who have weaker ties with others and 31 therefore tend to make decisions that only bind themselves" (Schwartz & Thompson, 1990, p.6). Cultural theory also maintains that identity is shaped by a second factor, namely the extent that social prescriptions constrain a person’s behavior (Dake, 1992). According to this ‘grid/group’ nomenclature, as cultural theory is sometimes called, social prescriptions (the grid dimensions) and group identity (the group dimension) give rise to distinctive myths of nature and specific types of rationality. This taxonomy elegantly captures the value domains previously described and orders them into a manageable number of ways of life. Specifically, decision-making strategies and how people manage themselves are reduced to five basic patterns of life: hierarchical, individualist, egalitarian, fatalist and autonomous (Rayner, 1986; Thompson, 1988; Thompson & James, 1989; Wynne, 1989). These relational forms, together with the cultural biases that justify them, are each hypothesized to engender shared representations of what does and does not constitute a management perspective (Douglas, 1975). Put another way, "adherence to a certain pattern of social relationships generates a distinctive way of looking at the world; adherence to a certain worldview Iegitimizes a corresponding type of social relations" (Thompson et al., 1990, p.1). Among all possible choices, those selected for consideration or dismissal serve (often intentionally) to strengthen one of these cultures and weaken the others. Hierarchically arranged groups are those stemming from high levels of stratified prescriptions (high grid) and strong group boundaries (high group) are hypothesized to foster the myth that nature is ‘perverse or tolerant’ (Dake, 1991). This myth holds nature to be robust, but only up to a point. Sustainable development is the rational environmental strategy in a hierarchical culture because this policy takes advantage of the 32 perceived resilience of nature, but respects the known limits (UNESCO, 1991). In this worldview, the limits of the ecosystem and hence appropriate resource conservation and development strategies, can only be proposed by certified decision makers or experts. The analogy here is that resource management is like a traditional family life where compliance to regulations is supposed to flow up the ranks of long-lasting institutions just as commands flow down (Dake, 1992). Egalitarian groups are those with strong ingroup/outgroup boundaries (high group) but with prescriptions that do not vary by rank and station (low grid) and believe the myth that nature is ‘fragile’ (Dake, 1991). Just as the experts-know-best approach to resource management justifies hierarchical social relations, so the egalitarian view that nature is ephemeral justifies the precautionary approach to management. Egalitarian groups are critical of the procedural rationality associated with hierarchy because they prefer approaches to management policies that foster equality of outcomes (Rayner, 1988b). Egalitarians are hypothesized to flame natural resource issues in ethical terms because this allows them to focus on the social and political dimensions and to criticize the institutions responsible for natural resource management (Dake, 1992). In its extreme form, egalitarianism calls for strict preservation of the environment (Wildavsky, 1991). The collective community that unites both the hierarchical and egalitarian defense and protection of the environment is the antithesis of the arguments put forth when individualist forms of social relations prevail. lndividualists are hypothesized to hold the myth of nature as benign, so that if people are released from artificial constraints (regulation and enforcement) there will be few limits to the abundance for all and this will more than compensate for any hazards created in the process. Deregulation is the 33 rational management strategy in low-grid/low-group cultures because individualists value decisions stemming from personal judgment rather than collective control (Rayner, 1988a). The term individuals in this context are social beings generating and stabilizing a form of social relations and institutions that defend their freedom to bid and bargain in self-regulated networks with few prescriptions (Thompson, 1992). Cultures of fatalism are those with high levels of prescription and with minimal collective participation and are hypothesized to hold the myth of nature as capricious. Fatalists may be those who have been excluded from the other ways of organizing social life; those who cannot compete successfully in markets, who cannot meet the minimum social standards of bounded and stratified groups and who cannot assemble the time, energy or resources required for political participation (Thompson et al., 1990). Equally plausible, fatalists may be those who simply want to be free from the disempowerment of influence from well-wishers (Dake, 1992). Either way, fatalists are hypothesized to construct a cultural bias that rationalizes isolation and resignation to stringent controls on their behavior (Mars, 1982). “Why bother?” is the rational for resource management strategies in this high-grid/low-group culture. Fatalists are thought to view life as a lottery in which no particular management strategy is best. Theirs’ is a capricious world, where they desire to be left alone and stay out of harms way. The fifth cultural pattern is autonomy - a largely asocial way of life and is not relevant to this research. Table 1 presents the four relevant worldviews. The literature on risk and cultural theory have empirical findings that are useful in understanding natural resource management decision-making. One finding is that those who hold an egalitarian bias (who value equity and lessen the distinctions between 34 Table 1: Grid/Group Nomenclature and Worldviews Social Prescriptions High Grid Low Grid §~ .3 High Group Hierarchical Egalitarian 8 5 Nature is Tolerant Nature is Fragile 0 2 Low Group Fatalists Individualist Nature is Capricious Nature is Benign Source: Dake 1992 people based on wealth, race, gender, authority, etc.), have been found to perceive the dangers associated with most technologies as great, and their attendant benefits as small (Dake, 1992). Among the largest empirical correlates of egalitarianism are concerns about environmental pollution, the dangers associated with nuclear energy and the threat of nuclear war (Dake, 1992). To clarify, it is not that cultural theory conceives that individualistic or hierarchical oriented people do not perceive environmental concerns or risks, just that they disagree with egalitarians about how these concerns should be ranked. For example, hierarchically oriented people correlate higher with concerns about the loss of respect for authority and other forms of insubordination, while individualism is more highly correlated with economic issues such as lack of a stable investment climate. Furthermore, this research on worldviews replicated previous findings that measures of a person’s worldviews are related to traditionally assessed personality traits and personal values as well as to social attitudes and policy preferences (Buss, Craik, & Dake, 1985; Buss, Criak, & Dake, 1986). For instance, in a. California sample, hierarchy was related to a cautious, moderate, unassuming personality style and to a highly conservative political orientation. Conversely, egalitarianism was associated with a less inhibited, more expressive and assertive way of behaving and with a more liberal political orientation (Dake, 1991). 35 The Evolution of the Fairness Heuristic as an Attitudinal Measure Some research suggests worldviews regarding natural resources become clearer when viewed through the lens of environmental ethics, (Wenz, 1988) and procedural and distributive justice (Syme & Nancarrow, 1997a; Syme & Nancarrow, 1997b; Syme et al., 1999). General worldviews become elevated to the level of an attitude when they focus on a specific attitudinal object and are viewed through the lenses of ethics and justice (Peterson, 1994; Syme et al., 1999). It has been shown that attitudes provide a much better predictor of behavior than values (Ajzen & F ishbein, 1980; Vaske & Donnelly, 1999) and in an effort understand the fairness heuristic it is necessary to explore the principles it is founded on. Environmental ethics have received considerable academic attention, particularly in the discipline of philosophy. Ethics can be defined as the “study or discipline which concerns itself with judgments of approval and disapproval, the tightness or wrongness, goodness or badness, virtue or vice, desirability or wisdom of actions, disposition, ends, objects, or states of affairs” (Runes, 1983, p.113). Environmental ethics deal more specifically with human conduct towards the natural environment. It is inevitable that humans interact with the natural environment. But “What ideas govern or structure this interaction?” and “What is the appropriate relationship between humans and nature?” For purposes of this study, environmental ethics are defined as the diversity of ideas driving human relationships with the natural environment (Wenz, 1988). Examples include stewardship of nature as a religious duty and the intrinsic rights of nature. As used in this study, environmental ethics are more focused constructs than values as they apply to human-environmental relationships generally rather than on specific natural objects. There is a rich literature in history, philosophy and other environmentally related 36 fields of study regarding environmental ethics. Much of this literature is reviewed in contemporary texts and this study relies on Wenz's “Environmental Justice” (Wenz, 1988) for structuring the attitudinal independent variable. Justice is a value judgment about the moral rightness of a person's fate (Furby, 1986). Treatment by other people or non-human forces (i.e. policies) is judged to be just if it meets the appropriate standards of what is morally correct. These standards are defined by supportive values. Justice supporting values differ within and across persons and context (Seligman, Syme, & Gilchrist, 1994) and are learned from cultural relations (Dake, 1992; Fiske & Taylor, 1990). Equity, the value that people should receive returns appropriate to their contribution and equality, the value that all people should receive the same return are two values that have received extensive research attention and indeed provided the foundation for the early research in water allocation. The value of fair . procedures has also received a share of research attention (e. g. Lind and Tyler, 1988). The literature on equity theory and research provides four key propositions about social behavior that are reflected in the fairness heuristic. They are: Proposition 1: Individuals will try to maximize their outcomes. The corollary to this is that if individuals perceive they can maximize their outcomes by behaving equitably, they will do so. Should they perceive they can maximize their outcomes by behaving inequitably, they will do so. Proposition [1: Groups can maximize collective rewards by evolving accepted systems for equitably apportioning resources (or costs) among members. Furthermore, groups will generally reward members who treat others equitably and generally punish (increase the costs for) members who treat others inequitably. Proposition [11: When individuals find themselves participating in inequitable relationships, they become distressed. The more inequitable the relationships, the more distressed individuals feel. 37 Proposition IV: Individuals who discover they are in an inequitable relationship will attempt to eliminate their distress by restoring equity. The greater the inequity that exists, the more distress they feel and the harder they will try to restore equity. (Source: Walster et al., 1978) Imbedded in the fairness heuristic questions are these four propositions. The intention is to elicit from the public how they feel equitable policies can be formulated while causing the least amount of distress. It seems appropriate at this point to review the history of ethics and justice considerations as they have been applied to water management. Initially in the area of water allocation, Pierce ( 1979) attempted to relate people's central values to their priorities for water allocation for the environment. This author found that adherence to Rokeach's (197 3) value of a ‘world of beauty’ resulted in a higher priority for water for environmental preservation. Conversely, support for a ‘comfortable life’ tended to be negatively associated with priorities for allocations for conservation. The relationships were modest however, and the values very general and perhaps too broad for an evaluation of specific allocation systems (Syme & Nancarrow, 1997a). Nevertheless, Pierce (1979) demonstrated that it was possible to empirically assess community values and their relationship to priorities for water allocation. In an initial attempt to apply more specific equity and procedural justice constructs to water allocation issues, Syme and Fenton (1993a) addressed community perceptions of equity and procedural justice in the context of groundwater allocation in Australia. The allocation issue included environmental as well as human uses. As an initial step, they attempted to replicate the measurement of Rasinki's (1987) proportionality and egalitarianism equity factors derived in the context of social welfare 38 policy. They then attempted to relate these to preferred allocation structures derived from Thibaut and Walker's (1975) early work on procedural justice. The results showed that Rasinski's (1987) general community factor of egalitarianism was able to be replicated and was strongly supported by the community sample. The concept of proportionality (or allocation based on returns appropriate to peOple's contribution) was less well defined and supported. Syme and Fenton's ( 1993a) work was successful in extrapolating some of the existing equity and justice theories in water allocation, but it was evident that the questions were too general to accurately reflect participants' views and therefore limited in its predictive ability. There was also a need for more precise questioning in the context of specific decision-making systems (Syme & Nancarrow, 1997a) than simply value orientations. Recall that Lind and Tyler’s (1988) results revealed that judgments of procedural fairness mediated the effects of outcome evaluations. Syme and Fenton's (1993) results would seem to support this finding and indeed concluded that while justice considerations provided some insights in relation to water allocation, they were far from sufficient for explaining the ethic or culture towards water management planning. Research in other disciplines has shown that quite sophisticated lay theories Operate in a variety of domains, such as economics and education (Fumham, 1988). Similarity, a range of philosophies and values may exist which influence attitudes towards the allocation of natural resources, for example the notion that nature is fragile (Bengston, 1994). The purpose of Syme et al.’s (1999) work in allocation was to establish the relationship, if any, between expectations of the planning process and perceptions of 39 ethics and equity as well as what philosophical questions the lay public were comfortable with (Syme & Nancarrow, 1992). Due to the failure to adequately capture the proportionality construct in the previously mentioned study, the philosophical questions posed to stakeholders were widened. This approach was warranted not only because the data seemed to indicate this need, but also because post survey interviews from the first study also suggested that two dimensions did not adequately represent the variety of Opinions on how to allocate water. Therefore, a wide variety of philosophical statements (initially 1 11) were posed to stakeholders to try and better capture how people thought about water allocation. These pertained to egalitarianism and proportionality from the first study and statements derived from Wenz's (1988) work "Environmental Justice". While some philosophies were harder to express in simple statements than others, the philosophies addressed included: Virtue Theory Water as a Common Good Water as an Economic Good Free Market Philosophy: Libertarianism Efficiency Principles Human Rights (Kant's Categorical Imperative, Positive Human Rights) Animal & Environmental Rights Utilitarianism as Hedonism Distributive Justice Certainty and Forecasting Cost Benefit (Willingness to Pay) Cost Benefit (Kaldor/Hicks Formulation) Procedural Justice (Rawls) Hare's Cost Benefit Philosophy Singer's Philosophy of Animal Rights Over subsequent studies in a variety of water allocation scenarios with a range of stakeholders, the number of statements was reduced to a manageable 25 to 30 assertions reflecting the ways people think about water management (Syme et al., 1999). The 40 fairness heuristic recognizes that separating procedural justice aspects of decision-making from those associated with the outcome and even subjective feelings of enjoyment from participation are quite difficult (Folger, 1996). Therefore, this version of the fairness heuristic reflects a better range of philosophical principles about water management that incorporates both procedural and moral concerns as well as those matters associated with outcomes of distributive justice. As a result Of these six studies, Syme and his fellow researchers (1999) confidently say the following: 0 Large portions of people believe in the ‘rights of the environment’ and its preservation for the range of uses for firture generations; 0 Fair decision-making processes are of paramount importance to community acceptance of water allocation decisions; 0 Water markets alone are not considered fair or acceptable processes for allocating or relocating water; 0 Economic arguments are of lesser importance to process considerations when deciding how water should be allocated or re-allocated; and o Efficiency of use is a major determinant of fairness of water allocation systems. Basic Categories of Environmental Policy and Implied Justice Concerns The justice challenge is to create policies that relate the benefits gained by polluting activities to the disadvantages or risks caused by them. One basic principle in ecological justice could be: equal proportions of benefits and costs from abuse of the common resources (Montada & Kals, 2000). This principle is certainly violated in cases of environmental racism, but more generally by every extemalization of costs caused by poflufion. Environmental costs are notoriously extemalized, which means that others, not those who cause them, bear the environmental damages and their costs: single citizens 41 within the community, the community as a whole, the state and other political jurisdictions (Montada & Kals, 2000). Externalized costs pose a serious justice problem. Those who cause the costs receive benefits without being charged with the costs. Inter-jurisdictional justice problems are raised when costs cross jurisdictional borders. Legal liability norms are a means to re-intemalize extemalized costs. Consequently, inter-jurisdictional liability norms are needed. However, application of these norms can be problematic as long as the cause of the damage is hard to validly identify. Furthermore, the amount and valuation of damage frequently remains open to question. There are six basic categories of environmental policy: (1) legal regulations, (2) regulation by tax, (3) subsidies, (4) pollution trading, (5) the establishment of human rights and (6) appeals to responsible actors. legal regulations aimed at the reduction of damages and risks, if strictly applied, are an effective means of environmental protection. At the same time they may reduce injustices in the distribution of costs and risk within and between groups (Montada & Kals, 2000; Tenbrunsel, Wade-Benzoni, Messick, & Bazerman, 1997). Since they are generally considered valid by society, they guarantee more equity (Montada & Kals, 2000). However, they also have costs and there may be losers (e. g. corporations that go bankrupt). One-sided strict jurisdictional anti-pollution norms may interfere with inter-jurisdictional competitiveness Of industries. Moreover, they can lead to restrictions of civil rights, which are only justifiable as a means to prevent dangerous risks or gross injustices (Montada & Kals, 2000). A simple example of the restriction of individual civil rights occurs when in efforts to protect water resources municipalities dictate to residents where they may and may not walk their pets. 42 Regulations by taxes and other charges, which are revenues for communities and the state, make production and consumption of goods that create pollution more expensive. This is the application of the ‘polluter pays principle’. Taxes and charges are meant to reduce the injustices of the associated benefits and costs. States or community revenues could be used for compensation of unjust ecological disadvantages as well as for preventative aims, such as subsidizing coo-friendly technologies or traffic systems, etc. (Tenbrunsel et al., 1997). Still, taxes and charges do not solve every justice problem. Those who have the resources are Often able to afford the higher taxes as in the case of gasoline. A sharp rise in taxes may mean economic ruin for corporations as well as private households. The subsidizing of eco-friendly alternatives for production, traffic, air conditioning and consumption is the third policy instrument. This supply-oriented policy is desirable because it does not restrict freedom rights, but it is negatively evaluated because it allocates costs to the community instead of those who have caused the damages and risks. Therefore, a combination of the first three policy instruments may be most effective and might at the same time prevent economic hazards (Montada & Kals, 2000). Another market-oriented policy is the allocation of emission rights. An example is the Clean Air Act 1990 that allows corporations a specific amount of risky emissions. The EPA is strongly promoting the use of effluent trading to achieve water quality objectives and standards (USEPA, 1996). Essential to this concept is the definition of emission rights as valuable and tradable goods (Montada & Kals, 2000). These types of policies may have a limiting as well as incentive functions to avoid emissions. 43 Unfortunately, the just allocation of emission rights is problematic. What will be the basis for allocation? Will it be the reduced emission rate through the use of best available technologies? or the mean emission rights for a particular business sector? or the number of employees within a corporation? or perhaps something else? The questions are endless as the problem becomes more complex if allocations are required between business sectors, generations and/or jurisdictions. The allocation of legal or constitutional rights to the natural environment might be a powerful measure to correct and prevent ecological damages and unjust distributions (Syme & Nancarrow, 1992; Syme & Nancarrow, 1997b) but it would also cause some problems. One is that the various constitutional rights are not consistent but partly conflict with each other. Ecological rights may interfere with freedom rights, with property rights or with the right to free gainfirl economic activities (Montada & Kals, 2000). Every newly established right such as the right to a safe ecology may restrict already established rights. Therefore, allocation of ecological rights does not provide consensual solutions. Rather, they legitimize claims that for the present may come into conflict not only with self-interests of other people, but also with their constitutional or legal rights. The last environmental policy category is appeals to responsible actors. This is probably the most common environmental policy in watershed planning at the present time. Appeals do not restrict freedom rights but neither do they prevent unjust distribution of benefits and costs. The segmentation of the community that complies with the appeals carries the burden and even contributes to the benefits of free-riding segments that do not comply (Montada & Kals, 2000). Therefore, those actors who are principally 44 willing to comply may feel unjustly disadvantaged in comparison to the free-riders and are demotivated when they become aware of the facts. PART II The Theories of Reasoned Action (TRA) and Planned Behavior (TPB) The theory of reasoned action (Ajzen & Fishbein, 1980; Fishbein & Ajzen, 1975) posits that behavioral intentions, which are immediate antecedents to behavior, are a function of salient information or beliefs about the likelihood that performing a particular behavior will lead to a specific outcome (Madden, Scholder Ellen, & Ajzen, 1992). Fishbein and Ajzen (1975) divide the beliefs antecedent of behavioral intentions into two conceptually distinct sets: behavioral and normative (Figure 2A). The behavioral beliefs are postulated to be the underlying influence on an individual’s attitude toward performing the behavior, whereas the normative beliefs influence the individual’s subjective norm about performing that behavior (Madden et al., 1992). Hence, information or salient beliefs affect intentions and subsequent behavior through attitudes and/or through subjective norms (Madden et al., 1992). As noted by Fishbein and Ajzen (1975), variables external to the model are assumed to influence intentions only to the extent that they affect either attitudes or subjective norms. The theory Of planned behavior (Ajzen, 1985) extends the boundary conditions of pure volitional control specified by the theory of reasoned action. This is accomplished by including beliefs regarding the possession of requisite resources and Opportunities for performing a given behavior (Madden et al., 1992). The more resources and opportunities individuals think they possess, the greater should be their perceived control over the 45 Behavioral Intentions Behavior Subjective Norms (A) Subjective Behavioral Behavior Norms Intentions ; if erceived Actual B eh av i 0 ral “..”-m”...- u._.‘..".......ououooou~ounusuouuuno-uu 9-00 B ehavi o ral Control Control (B) Figure 3: Path Models for Theory of Reasoned Action (A) and Theory of Planned Behavior (B) Source: Ajzen, 2002 46 behavior (Ajzen, 1985). That is, when people have complete control over the behavior, intentions alone should be sufficient to predict the behavior and perceived behavioral control will make no significant contribution. In contrast, when a behavior is not under complete volitional control, perceived behavioral control (to the extent that it is accurate) provides important information that should add to the predictability of the model (Madden et al., 1992). Figure 23 presents the theory of planned behavior. Perceived behavioral control is included as an exogenous variable that has both a direct effect on behavior and indirect effect on behavior through intentions. The indirect effect is based on the assumption that perceived behavioral control has motivational implications for behavioral intentions. When people believe they have little control over performing the behavior because of a lack of requisite resources, then their intentions to perform the behavior may be low even if they have favorable attitudes and/or subjective norms concerning performance of the behavior (Eden, 1993; Madden et al., 1992). Bandura, Adams, Hardy, and Howells (1980) have provided empirical evidence that people’s behavior is strongly influenced by the confidence in their ability to perform the behavior. The theory of planned behavior specifies that for behaviors not completely under volitional control, perceived behavioral control will add to the prediction of behavior over and above the effect of behavioral intentions (Ajzen & Fishbein, 1977; Sutton, 1998). The direct path from perceived behavioral control to behavior is assumed to reflect the actual control an individual has over performing the behavior. Since the model used for the presented research does not directly measure behavior, this link cannot be tested and further discussion is therefore unwarranted. 47 The Behavioral Intention - Behavior Relationship Sutton (1998) showed in his meta-analyses that when behavior was predicted from intentions only, the product-moment correlation ranged between .44 and .62 (i.e. explaining between 19% and 38% of the variance). In Cohn's (1992) terms, these would all be described as medium or large effects. There are several reasons that may cause poor predictive power; Sutton (1998) cites nine possibilities, but of particular interest to this researcher is the principle of correspondence (Ajzen & Fishbein, 197 7; Fishbein & Ajzen, 1975) since it can be partially controlled for in the survey instrument. The principle of correspondence states that in order to maximize predictive power, the predictor (attitude) and the criterion (intentions) should be measured at the same level of specification or generality (Ajzen & Fishbein, 1977; Ajzen & Fishbein, 1980; Sutton, 1998). The measure should be matched with respect to four components, action, target, time and context. There is substantial empirical evidence to support this idea (Ajzen, 1991; van den Putte, 1993) and consequently this study uses theories surrounding environmental philosophies and justice to guide the formulation of the independent variable based upon value orientations and general attitudes (see environmental justice and the fairness heuristic section). Attitudes One of the first attempts at quantifying the strength of the relationship between attitude and environmental behavior was Hines, Hungerford and Tomera’s (1986) meta- analysis. For the purposes of their study, the attitudinal variable included those factors that dealt with the individual’s feelings with regard to particular aspects of the environment or objects related to the environment (Hines et al., 1986). Therefore, their 48 categorization of attitude included assessments of general attitude towards the environment as well as more specific attitudes such as those towards the energy crisis, unleaded gasoline and taking environmental action. NO distinction was made between affective and cognitive components of attitudes. F ifty-one outcome measures on the attitude—behavior relationship were coded. Meta-analysis of the full set of these studies resulted in a corrected correlation coefficient of .347 (SD = .224). Further examination of the data was conducted in an effort to determine the nature of the attitudes under study. Consequently, forty-two of the attitude studies coded dealt with attitudes towards the environment while nine studies were concerned with attitudes towards taking action. A slightly stronger relationship was detected between attitude towards action and behavior (r = .377, SD = .145) than was observed between attitude towards the environment in general and behavior (r = .338, SD = .243). Contrary to most studies when actual behaviors were assessed, the correlations were higher than when behavior was self-reported (r = .427, SD = .290). Similarly when the individuals in the studies had ties to environmental organizations, the correlations were higher (r = .593, SD = .273). One will note that the Hines, Hungerford and Tomera’s (1986) meta-analysis reported on the attitude-behavior relationship and did not make the distinction between behavioral intentions and behaviors. It is also somewhat dated. Therefore, it seems prudent to conduct a review of more recent environmental studies and where possible to examine issues surrounding water management. Although used as a measure of environmental attitudes, beliefs, values and worldviews, the New Environmental Paradigm Scale has been widely used over the past 49 two decades. It has been used most often with samples of the general public, but it has also been used with samples of specific sectors such as farmers (Dunlap, Van Liere, Mertig, & Jones, 2000). In general, the studies have found a relatively strong endorsement of NEP beliefs across the various samples (i.e. correlations similar to the Hines et al., 1986 study above). Both the ability to predict and identify groups from a sample are forms of criterion validity. In studies of environmental interest groups, NEP studies have consistently found that environmentalists score higher on the NEP scale than the general public (Edgell & Nowell, 1989; Pierce, Steger, Steel, & Lovrich, 1992; Widegren, 1998). Similarly, despite the difficulty of predicting behaviors from general attitudes and beliefs, numerous studies have found significant relationships between the NEP Scale and various types of behavioral intentions as well as both self-reported and observed behaviors (Shultz & Oskamp, 1996; Stem et al., 1995). Therefore, the overall evidence suggests that the NEP and other general attitudinal scales possess criterion validity. Turning toward environmental studies that focus on a specific environmental attitudinal object (e.g., forests), it can be shown that the correlation between attitude and intentions not only increases, but that the correlation between attitude and intention is greater than that between attitude and measures of behavior. Also, as expected from the general studies, these increases are especially significant if the study pOpulation has an interest in the attitudinal Obj ect such as a hunter might have about deer. For example, Fulton, Manfredo and Lipscomb (1996) found that attitudes mediated the relationship between value orientations and behavioral intentions for 1,202 adult residents of Colorado when asked about hunting/fishing and viewing of wildlife. They found the path 50 between hunting/fishing attitudes and intention to be significant (,6 = .79, t = 11.73, p _<_ .001) as was the path between wildlife viewing attitudes and intentions (,8 = .56, t = 8.84, p _<_ .001). In both cases the paths between value orientations and intentions was not significant. In two other studies, one by Hrubes, Ajzen and Daigle (2001) on hunting attitudes and behavior and one by Vaske and Donnelly (1999) on wildlands preservation, similar results to the Fulton et al., (1996) study were found. Of the 727 people mailed a survey from a list of those who purchased hunting licenses in Vermont in 1997, 395 predominantly white males (73%) showed a significant relationship between attitudes and intentions (fl = .58, p 5_ .01). With regard to wildland preservation, 960 residences (53% response rate) living along the Front Range region of Colorado also demonstrated a significant relationship between attitudes and intentions by indicating a pro-wildland preservation voting intention (fl = .94, p < .001, R2 = 88%). There are just two studies on water management using the theory Of planned behavior or theory of reasoned action. In a study on whether to adopt micro-irrigation techniques on 44 strawberry farms in the State Of Florida, Lynne (1995) found several interesting relations. First, it was found that perceived control was important in explaining both the decision of whether or not to adOpt micro-irrigation techniques and how much to invest in conservation technology. Second, it was found that the significance of the subjective norm variable implied farmers are influenced by community norms for water conserving behavior and that individuals who are more influenced by the community will be more likely to adOpt and will adOpt more intensively. The researchers maintain that the significance for perceived control, which is 51 another type of community influence, showed that coercive control (which would reduce perceived control) could be counter productive. That is, it would not only slow the move to becoming an adopter, but also reduce the intensity of investment in conserving technology. They also found that actual control was significant. The fact that both perceived behavioral control and actual control added significant explanatory power also suggested that measuring only financial influence on investment is insufficient. The second study by Luzar and Cosse (1998) was on Louisiana well owners’ willingness to pay for changes in state level water quality. They found that the attitudinal variables significantly enhanced the explanatory power of their willingness to pay models at both the state and individual levels. The state wide model improved from R2 = .08 to R2 = .19 with the attitude variables and at the local level from R2 = .10 to R2 = .22 at the individual level (both at a 90% level of confidence). Additionally, the subjective norm variable was positive and significant in both models. Several other variables were also found relevant to the Luzar and Cosse model. Ownership of a private source of drinking water was positively associated with willingness-tO-pay for improvements in water quality. The presence of young children in the respondent’s household was positively and significantly associated with willingness- to-pay for changes in water quality. Respondents with higher income were not only willing to pay, but able to pay for changes in water quality. There was a non-linear significant positive relationship between an individual’s age and education and willingness-to—pay. Not statistically significant were explanatory variables indicating occupation and gender. 52 Personal Responsibility Geller (1995) has proposed that certain psychological states or expectancies affect the propensity for individuals to actively care for the safety or health of others. The term active caring refers to individuals who care enough about a particular problem or about other people to implement an intervention process in attempts to make a beneficial difference (Geller, 1995). By this definition, active caring is equivalent to the personal responsibility variable in this study and is considered a social norm. Allen and Ferrand’s (1999) study of college students’ tests Geller’s hypothesis that actively caring is an important mediator leading to environmental concerns and action. They found no evidence of a direct relation between personal control and environmentally friendly behaviors. Instead, and consistent with Geller’s (1995) model, sympathy (i.e. the proxy used for active caring) was found to mediate the relation between personal control and total environmental behaviors. Specifically, the path from sympathy to total environmentally friendly behavior was significant (,6 = .39, t(94) = 3.47, p = .25), but the direct path between personal control and total environmentally friendly behavior was not significant (,8 = .14, t(94) = 1.15, p . .25). While Allen and Ferrand’s (1999) results support Geller’s hypothesis, Hwang et al.’s (2000) study on forest management found only a small relationship between personal responsibility and intent to act (,8 = .20, #436). A parallel line of research examining the relation between altruistic social norms and environmentally responsible behavior is also consistent with Geller’s (1995) views. Specifically, Herberlein (1972) maintained that protecting the environment is perceived as a moral and altruistic issue because environmental damage has negative consequences for others. As a result, Herberlein (1972) suggested that Schwartz’s (1970; 1977) moral 53 norm activation model of altruism could be used to predict environmentally friendly behaviors. Schwartz’s model predicts that altruistic behavior results when a moral norm is activated. This activation occurs when an individual becomes aware that their behavior has possible negative consequences for others and is willing to take personal responsibility for the other’s well being. Therefore, Herberlein (1972) suggested that individuals should act to protect the environment in situations similar to those that elicit altruism: when an individual is both aware that their actions can have negative consequences for the environment and feels personally responsible for these consequences. Several authors (e. g. Guagnano, 1995; Hooper & Neilsen, 1991; Van Liere & Dunlap, 1978) have acted on Heberlein’s suggestion and have demonstrated that Schwartz’s (1970, 1977) model can be used to predict environmentally fiiendly behavior. For example, Van Liere and Dunlap (1978) found that individual rural residents who were both aware of the negative environmental consequences of burning yard waste and who also accepted responsibility for the environmental consequences of burning were least likely to burn yard waste. Similarly, but more globally, Guagnano (1995) found that residents’ awareness of the environmental consequences of their actions and their willingness to take responsibility for these actions combined to predict willingness to take action to protect the environment on a range of issues. These findings are consistent with Geller’s model in that they demonstrate environmentally friendly behaviors are at least partly a function of altruism. 54 Locus of Control The locus of control (LOC) factor in the model represents one of the most extensively researched variables. Locus of Control refers to a general belief regarding the location of forces controlling an individual’s life (i.e. internal vs. external factors). Persons with an internal locus of control believe they normally have personal control over important life events as a result of their knowledge, skill and abilities (Geller, 1995). In contrast, people with an external locus of control believe factors like luck, chance or fate have significant influence in their lives (Rotter, 1966). Therefore, people with an external LOC generally expect to have less personal control over the pleasant and unpleasant consequences they experience than do people with an internal LOC. The use of LOC for the proposed model of community action would appear justified since it encompasses both external and internal components of an individual. There exists an intimate relationship between LOC and personal responsibility (Eden, 1993). This is because individuals focus on actionable responsibility, not solely on moral responsibility (Eden, 1993). To be actionable, an individual must perceive the internal control they can maintain over the outcomes of their behavior. That is, they have a strong belief in their own efficacy. Therefore, the identification of pro-environmental behavior is not only based upon it being responsible, but also possible and efficacious. Actionable responsibility is curtailed by its context in that behavior is only morally required within its efficacious range, which is usually an immediate one. Rather than suggesting mere self-interest, the immediacy of responsibility points to individuals being necessarily preoccupied with immediate issues (O'Riordan & Rayner, 1991). Beyond the immediate, efficacy is weakened which in turn weakens perceived responsibility (Eden, 1993). It becomes pointless to ascribe responsibility to the self to 55 undertake behavior that has no effect. There is an implicit utilitarian ethos to this reasoning in that responsibility is not limited because of morality alone, but functionally because of its efficacy in curtailing environmental damage (Eden, 1993). This supports Schwartz’s (1970) emphasis on awareness of the consequences (see below) of a person’s actions as contributing towards responsible behavior. Therefore, context is influential in that it affects how efficacy is perceived. Efficacy is reinforced (or not) by societal norms underlying the need for setting individual pro-environmental behaviors within social circumstances (Eden, 1993). In recent years, there have been numerous studies aimed at exploring the relationship between locus of control and environmentally responsible behavior (Hines et al., 1986; Sia, Hungerford, & Tommera, 1985/86; Smith-Sebasto, 1992). As a result of this work, it has been suggested that instruments created to measure LOC as it relates to a specific situation or behavior should yield more reliable results than a generic scale (Lefcourt, 1982; Phares, 1976; Rotter, 1975). This would suggest if researchers wish to create a locus of control instrument that would allow for precise predictions of environmentally responsible behavior, they should construct items with referents to specific environmentally responsible behaviors or actions (Smith-Sebasto & Fortner, 1994). Therefore, the instrument designed for this research was adapted from the Environmental Action Internal Control Index (EAIC) (Smith-Sebasto & Fortner, 1994) to the context of BMP implementation for watershed management. The EAICI was chosen because evaluation of previous instruments concluded that their internal consistency and validity were questionable (Smith-Sebasto & Fortner, 1994). The development of the EAICI started with the identification of examples of pro- 56 environmental behaviors from numerous published sources in the popular press. Items that eventually became part of the instrument were those that should have a high behavior-attitude relationship (Ajzen & Fishbein, 1977, 1980). The EAICI instrument is comprised of 28 items of which six have been adapted to the context of watershed management for the purposes of this study. The Smith-Sebasto and Fortner (1994) study of undergraduate university students found the correlation between LOC (as measured by the EAICI) and environmentally responsible behavior was modestly positive (r = .33) at the .01 level of significance (Smith-Sebasto & Fortner, 1994). Through discriminant analysis they also found the EAICI accurately classified individuals self-reported environmental behavior in almost 82% of the cases. Further discriminant analysis showed that the EAICI could also accurately classify individuals on the basis of perceived knowledge or skill at the use of environmental action strategies (Smith-Sebasto & Fortner, 1994). Knowledge In natural resource management, the level of factual knowledge has been identified as an external variable that links values and attitudes (Tarrant, Bright, & Cordell, 1997). Although the effect of knowledge on attitudes is not conclusive, there have been numerous studies suggesting a link between the two. For instance, Bright & Manfredo (1997) concluded that individuals with higher knowledge levels have more positive attitudes than those with low levels of knowledge. Fortner and Lyon (1985) found a similar result when they studied the effects of viewing a Cousteau television special on attitudes, although the attitudinal change appeared temporary (lasting only approximately 2 weeks). Meanwhile, Steel et al. (1990) found that policy-relevant 57 knowledge was associated with perceptions of risk in the Great lakes for Canadians but not Americans. There are two possible explanations for this ambiguity between studies: (1) the appropriate knowledge component for predicting attitude has not been identified (Boerschig & DeYoung, 1993; McKenzie-Mohr, Nemiroff, Beers, & Desmarais, 1995) and/or (2) that context matters (Ajzen & Fishbein, 1977). Despite these inconclusive findings, knowledge enhances the association between general environmental value orientations (and by extension worldviews) and environmental policy preferences by increasing the explained variance (Pierce, Lovrich, Tsurutani, & Abe, 1989; Steel et al., 1990; Tarrant et al., 1997). Thus, it has been included in the model. Environmental knowledge can be categorized into three levels: (1) knowledge about the issues, (2) knowledge about the action strategy and (3) action skill (Boerschig & DeYoung, 1993; Hines et al., 1986). Interestingly, these different levels of knowledge are considered to influence the responsible environmental behavior in different ways. For example, Hungerford and Volk (1990) categorized environmental behavior related variables into categories: entry-level, ownership-level and empowerment-level. They suggested that knowledge about general concepts is considered an important variable for entry-level, in depth knowledge about issues for the ownership-level and knowledge about action strategies and skills for the empowerment level. This suggests that depending upon the context of the research, different types of knowledge should be elicited fi'om respondents depending on the study goals. Traditionally, researchers in the field of environmental education have claimed they can change behavior by making peOple more knowledgeable about environmental issues. The reasoning is that if one has more knowledge about the environment, the 58 awareness level should be higher, thus producing a more favorable attitude (Hungerford & Volk, 1990). Individuals with a more favorable attitude in turn will be more predisposed toward environmentally friendly behavior (see attitude section). Yet, more environmental knowledge does not necessarily mean an increase in environmentally responsible actions. A major difficulty in imparting environmental literacy lies in the simple fact that research has not yet satisfactorily identified the knowledge components that are precursors to responsible environmental behavior (Sivek & Hungerford, 1989). Nevertheless, many researchers have used the change of knowledge as an effective variable for explaining the change in environmentally responsible behavior (Sivek & Hungerford, 1989; Smith-Sebasto & Fortner, 1994). The Smith-Sabasto and Fortner (1994) study of 853 undergraduate university students found several interesting relationships between knowledge, locus of control (LOC) and environmentally responsible behavior. A correlation of .23 (p > .01) between perceived knowledge of and skill at using environmental action strategies and LOC was found, indicating a low positive relationship. A discriminant analysis was performed between the scores on knowledge and LOC in an effort to assess whether knowledge could predict an individual’s LOC orientation. Sixty-one percent of the grouped cases were correctly classified. When a discriminant analysis between LOC and knowledge was performed in an effort to predict an individual’s knowledge level from their LOC score, eighty percent of the grouped cases were correctly classified. Hwang et al. (2000) found a more modest correlation between knowledge and LOC (13 = .02 ). They attributed this modest relation to their using an instrument designed to test general environmental 59 knowledge as opposed to more specific questions on knowledge or skills in their subject matter (i.e. forest use). Sabasto-Smith and Fortner’s (1994) study also found a moderately positive correlation (r = .483, .01 level of significance) between knowledge and environmentally responsible behavior, while Hines et al. (1986) found a correlation of r =. 185 in their meta-analysis. These findings are similar to several other studies (Sia et al., 1985/86; Sivek & Hungerford, 1989). However, the correlation between scores does not necessarily indicate a direct relationship. In fact it is assumed by many researchers that knowledge will influence attitude, which in turn will affect behavior (Hungerford & Volk, 1990; Newhouse, 1990). Results from the Hwang et al. (2000) study support this. They found a small relationship between knowledge and attitude (,6 = .09) and no significant relation between knowledge and intent to act. As was the case previously, the magnitude of this relationship could also have been affected by the use of a general knowledge instrument and that result should improve by testing other levels of knowledge such as those surrounding action skills and strategies (Boerschig & DeYoung, 1993; Hines et al., 1986). Regardless of the magnitude of the relationship, this finding still indicates that attitude is a mediator variable between knowledge and intention to act. Hamilton (1986), Eden (1993) and others have suggested there exists a relationship between environmental knowledge and personal responsibility. This relationship is also intuitive; the more one knows about a subject the more likely they will feel a sense of personal responsibility, especially if the subject directly impacts on them. Beckwith and Rayl (2002) found a partial correlation coefficient of. 1744 (p=.005) between general environmental knowledge and environmental responsibility among 60 college students. Also, Hwang, et al. (2000) found a small relationship between knowledge and personal responsibility (r = .03) in their study of forest management. Demographic Variables The extensive literature on environmental attitudes, age, education, urban residence and political ideology indicates that socio-demographic factors are found to have a consistent, substantially significant association with environmental attitude (Christianson & Arcury, 1992; Van Liere & Dunlap, 1980). The conclusions of these factors indicates that younger, better educated, urban, liberal individuals are more concerned about the environment and have more positive attitudes toward the environmental movement. Other factors that have weak or inconsistent relationships to environmental attitude include gender, income and occupational prestige (Christianson & Arcury, 1992; Samdahl & Robertson, 1989). Income, education, gender (particularly male), and environmental attitude have a consistent positive association with public environmental knowledge (Arcury & Johnson, 1987; Arcury, Johnson, & Scollay, 1986; Lovrich, Pierce, Tsurutani, & Abe, 1986; Pierce et al., 1989). Age, being liberal and exposure to sources of information such as television news programs are less consistently correlated with environmental knowledge. Lovrich et al. (1986) also found that the perceived seriousness of a water problem to be positively associated with environmental knowledge and environmental knowledge to be positively associated with support for protective water policy measures. 61 Predicting and Explaining Intentions and Behavior Using TRA & TPB This section summarizes the results of meta-analyses on the theories of TRA/TPB to determine the percentage of variance explained. These reviews vary greatly in terms of the number and type of studies included and the sophistication of the meta-analytic methods used. Table 2 presents this summary. Table 2: Summary of Findings From Meta-Analyses of the TRA and TPB Regression of attitudes and social norms or attitudes, social norms and perceived behavioral Effect Size control on behavioral intentions Reviewer R R2 Farley etpal. (1981) .71 .50 Sheppard et al. (1988) .66 .44 Azjen (1991) .71 .50 van den Putte (1993) .68 .46 , Conner & Arrnitage (1998) 63 .40 Adapted from: Sutton, 1998, p. 1320 The findings for behavioral intentions show reasonable consistency with multiple correlations ranging from between .63 to .71. This accounts for between 40% and 50% of the variance. For intentions, the two theories are typically explaining no more than 50% of the variance. This seems disappointing in view of the fact that in the vast majority of the studies, intentions and its predictors are measured at the same time on the questionnaire using similar items; conditions that should maximize predictive power (Sutton, 1998). There are a number of different standards of comparison that can be used in evaluating the percentage of variance explained. Neither the TRA nor TPB fare well by this standard. In practice, however, the maximum percentage of variance that can be explained in a real application is often substantially less than 100. For example Table 3 shows five effect size measures that can be calculated on a simple randomized control experiment on a smoking intervention program. 62 Table 3: Examples of How Different Effect Size Can Give a Different Impression Randomized control trial of a new treatment for smoking Condition 1 N l Succeed | Fall Intervention 100 70 30 Control 100 30 70 Difference in success rates = 70 —30 = 40 Odds ratio = (70 x 70)/(30 x 30) = 5.4 Relative success = 70/30 = 2.3 Product-moment r (phi-coefficient) = .40 Percentage of variance grplained = 16 MnfiWN-A Adapted from: Sutton, 1998, p. 1323 The difference measure shows that the intervention improved the success rate by 40 percentage points. The odds of successfully quitting smoking were over five times higher in the intervention condition, compared with the control condition. The relative success rate shows that the intervention more than doubled the chances of successfully quitting. All of these measures suggest that the new treatment had a substantial and clinically useful effect. However, the percentage of variance explained in the dichotomous independent variable was 16 %, which seems unimpressive. Therefore, it can be concluded that the 40% - 50% of variance explained in the various meta-analyses 0f TBA/TPB are indeed large effect sizes (Cohn, 1992). Summary The beginning of this literature review proposed a theoretical foundation for the StUdy of attitudes and their effect on intention to act. It began with presenting a hierarchy 0fPsychological variables beginning with values and explained three theories of how values might operate in decision-making. Progressing up the hierarchy, the next section focused on value-orientations and beliefs and showed how these were founded in lndlvIdUal values and were relevant to worldviews. Worldviews were shown to be 63 general attitudes and represented value orientations within a context. Four relevant worldviews were presented. Drawing from cultural theory, the worldviews were hierarchical (high group/high grid), egalitarian, (high group, low grid), individualists (low, group, low grid) and fatalists (high grid, low group). The last section in part one dealt with attitudes and the fairness heuristic. This section discussed the philosophical tenants underlying this research instrument and presented its evolutionary development. Part II of the literature review began by presenting the theory of planned behavior, which was the underling theory for the study design. The theory of planned behavior defined the scope of the research and defined the study variables. Having defined the variables, the remainder of the literature concerned itself with reviewing the relationship between the study variables knowledge, locus of control, personal responsibility, attitudes and intent to act. Table 4 summarizes these findings. Clearly, from this summary table it is apparent that the psychological variables of attitude, locus of control and personal responsibility are the most important variables in influencing behavior and behavioral intentions. Knowledge is important, but indirectly and its effects on behavior and intentions appear to be mediated by the other psychological variables. lastly, demographic variables are poor predictors of behavior and behavioral intentions. 64 Table 4: Summary of Findings on the Strength of the Relationships Between Psychological Variables Variable Beta Relationships correlation Weights Study Intention - Behavior .44 - .62 Sutton (1998) Attitude - Behavior .35 Hines et al. (1986) — meta analysis 17 Beckwith & Rayl (2002) - ' undergraduates . . .56 - Fulton, et al. 1996) - Ammdes “ Imemmns .79 hunting/wildlife viewing .18 .09 Hwang et al. (2000) — forest .58 Hrubes et al. (2001) - hunting .94 Vaske & Donnely (1999) - wildland .28 _ .47 Lazar & Cosse (1998) - water quality Attitude - Responsible .10 Hwang et al. (2000) .43 Beckwith & Rayl (2002) Responsible - Intention .10 Hwang et al. (2000) Responsible - Behavior .39 Geller (1995) — safety & health .33 Hines et al. (1986) 44 Allen & Ferrand (1999) — ° undergraduates .36 Beckwith & Ray] (2002) LOC -Responsible .17 .13 Hwang et al. (2000) LOC - Behavior .31 Allen & Ferrand (1999) 33 Smith—Sabasto (1994) - ' undergraduates .36 Hines et al. (1986) LOC - Intent .20 Hwang et al. (2000) .37 Hines et al. (1986) LOC - Attitude .39 .39 Hwang et al. (2000) Knowledge - Behavior .05 Hwang et al. (2000) .30 Hines et al. (1986) .17 Beckwith & Rayl (2002) Knowledge - Attitude .09 .09 Hwang et al. (2000) .18 Beckwith & Rayl (2002) Knowledge -— LOC .02 .02 Hwang et al. (2000) .23 Smith—Sabasto (1994) Knowledge - .03 .02 Hwang et al. (2000) .24 Beckwith & Rayl (2002) Education - Behavior .18 Hines et al. (1986) .48 Smith-Sabasto (1994) Age - Behavior -. 15 Hines et al. (1986) Income —— Behavior .16 Hines et a1. (1986) 65 CHAPTER III: METHODOLOGY Study Design This study attempts to discover people’s worldviews, attitudes and the intent to support the implementation of best management practices based upon selected antecedents. The data collection method is a mail survey. A mail survey was chosen in order to secure a sufficient sample size to be able to infer characteristics, attitudes, and behaviors for the entire watershed population (Babbie, 1990). There was also a need to be able to support policy Options quantitatively. Background The study was conducted in Sycamore Creek, a sub-watershed of the Red Cedar River, which is part of the central Michigan portion of the Grand River watershed. The watershed drainage area is approximately 67,740 acres and is located in the center of Ingham County (NRCS, 1990). The primary land use in the southern half of the watershed is agriculture with one major population center (the City of Mason). The northern half of the watershed covers part of each of the cities of Holt and lansing. Also, approximately 1,500 acres of Michigan State University farmland is located in the northern part of the watershed (NRCS, 1990). Several problems have been identified in the Sycamore Creek watershed. The major types of pollutants to be controlled are sediment from soil erosion, phosphorous fertilizers, nitrate fertilizers and agricultural pesticides (NRCS, 1990). These pollutants cause sedimentation and turbidity problems, nuisance algae growth and 66 Ingham County L . D 811*me * East . Lansing W % (K L; ‘W’illiamston [:33] . ‘ Webherville Sycamore ..p Crook } Watershed . WK 5‘] * P Mason Z [El m. D Mchigan Figure 4: Map of Sycamore Creek Watershed in Relation to Ingham County, MI Population and Sample groundwater contamination (N RCS, 1990). These types of problems are frequently addressed by the instillation of best management practices by property owners. The study sample was drawn from Ingham County’s 2001 tax database. There are 67 21,801 properties on the tax roll within Sycamore Creek watershed. Since the independent variable of the study is individual attitudes, the research focused only on the 16,991 residential and agricultural properties. The categories of residential and agricultural land were determined using a code assigned to each property indicating its land use. The categories of commercial, industrial and developable were excluded. These required codes are standardized for the State of Michigan. Each of the residential prOperties was assigned a computer generated random number. It was determined through the use of a standard sample size formula (Babbie, 1990) that there needed to be 376 respondents in order to achieve a 95% confidence level and 639 respondents to achieve a 99% confidence level. An initial mailing of a letter of introduction and explanation consisted of 1,750 property owners. The names of those residents whose letters were not able to be delivered were removed from the mailing list for the first survey. A total Of 1,650 prOperty owners were mailed questionnaires with the hope there would be sufficient respondents to achieve the 99% confidence level. The sample was comprised of 1576 randomly selected residential properties and all 74 agriculture properties. To be included in the sample, a property owner had to reside either within the Sycamore Creek watershed or the greater Red Cedar watershed. Due to of the small number Of agricultural properties, there was no attempt to separate them out as a unit of analysis. After removing those questionnaires that remained undeliverable or were either refusals or blank returns, there remained 1,542 usable surveys. Of these 1,542 questionnaires, 608 usable surveys were returned for an overall response rate of 39%. It should be noted that the response rate might have been higher had not the mailing occurred shortly after the events of September 11, 2001 and also which pushed the study’s mailing into the holiday season. 68 Instrumentation The questionnaire was constructed from both original research material and components from other research instruments. Dillman’s (2000) Tailored Design Method was used to guide both construction and administration of the mail survey. The questionnaire had seven sections: (1) Use and Thoughts About the Red Cedar River; (2) Water Quality Concerns; (3) Best Management Practices; (4) BMP Combinations; (5) Choice Experiment and Implementation Information; (6) Attitudes and Beliefs; and (7) Demographics. Only part of section 1 and sections 2, 3, 5,6 and 7 were used for this study. Initially, three focus groups were conducted with local experts in an effort to understand the issues surrounding BMPs and to establish content validity. The scientific panel, as it came to be known, was drawn from organizations such as the Michigan Department of Environmental Quality (MDEQ), the National Resource Conservation Service (NRCS), Michigan State University Extension (MSUE), various departments at Michigan State University and the Soil Conservation District. The information collected in these focus groups helped determine survey content and structure in general and more specifically, the policy choices presented in the questionnaire (section 5). The questionnaire was designed using Hines, Hungerford and Tomera’s (1986) proposed model of responsible environmental behavior (Figure 1 is presented below again for convenience). This model was chosen for two reasons: (1) it contained all the elements required by the Theory of Planned Behavior, but (2) it was not a path analytic model. It was felt that the lack of previous research on attitudes toward water resource management in a specific context would make it likely that any method reliant upon the tight definition of the factors (as needed in a path analytic model) would fail. That is, 69 Situational Factors Action Skills Knowledge of } Action Strategies Knowledge 7 V of Issues I t ti Responsible . 11 en 0“ Environmental Attitudes —’ t0 ACt Behavior Locus of Control \ Personality Factors Personal Responsibility Figure 5: Hines, Hungerford and Tomera’s (1986) Proposed Model of Responsible Environmental Behavior even if the factor scales were valid and reliable in a previous context, it does not guarantee success in a new context due to the restructuring of the factors towards a new attitudinal object by individual respondents. This study concerns itself with the intention to act and its antecedents. Furthermore, since there is not an action skill required by the respondent in order to support (or not) the implementation of BMPs by municipalities, this factor is superfluous and not included in the model. Specific situational factors were not able to be totally captured by the survey instrument although there was considerable effort to establish 70 context. Consequently, the final model used in this study is presented in Figure 1. The overall questionnaire design followed the pattern of eliciting general information from the respondent, educating them about the current problems and possible solutions and then asking them what they wish to do about the problem. Knowledge of Action Strategies Knowledge of Issues Intention to Act Attitudes \ Social and Locus of Personality Control Factors Personal { / Responsibility Figure 1: Proposed Model of Responsible Environmental Behavioral Intentions Based on Hines, Hungerford and Tomera (1986) Knowledge of the Issues and of Action Strategies An index of Knowledge of the Issues was created through summing responses to five dichotomous, yes (I) or no (0) questions (Q6-Q10). This section contained questions such as whether the respondent knew about stormwater overflow plans and non-point source pollution. A Knowledge of Action Strategies index was similarly created through summing responses to six yes (1) no (2) or don’t know (0) questions (Q11-Ql6). This 71 section contained questions on whether they knew about each of six structural BMPs used to control stormwater runoff. The use of a dichotomous variable is consistent with other studies measuring knowledge (Reading, Clark, & Kellert, 1994, Fortner & Lyon, 1985 #294; Steel et al., 1990) with the reasoning being that the respondent either possesses the knowledge or they do not and the higher the score on the index, the greater one knows. Those respondents indicating “Don’t Know” in the knowledge of action strategies index were included in the “NO” category for the analysis. Attitude Construction of the Attitude factors was developed from items contained in the fairness heuristic instrument as reported by Syme, Nancarrow and McCreddin (1999). The attitude section consisted of twenty closed ended questions (out of thirty one possible questions from the original instrument). A five-point Likert agree/disagree response format was used. Questions range from inquiring whether people believed the “. . .environment had the same right to water as peOple” to “. . .whether those people upstream had a moral responsibility to look after those downstream”. Locus of Control Several locus of control (LOC) instruments have been developed to measure the relationship between LOC and environmentally responsible behavior. In their 1994 article, Smith-Sabasto and Fortner demonstrated the internal consistency and validity of these instruments were questionable. Their research compared the Environmental Action Internal Control Index (EAICI) to other LOC instruments (e. g. Brown LOC, Index Control Index and Need for Control Scale) and found that the EAICI was more highly 72 correlated with environmentally fiiendly behavior than other instruments. Furthermore, the correlation coefficients between the various scales studied lent support to the EAICI’s convergent and discriminant validity. Therefore, the EAICI was chosen to assess individual levels of locus of control on managing the Sycamore Creek watershed (Smith- Sebasto & Fortner, 1994). Internal consistency of the original scale is reported at .92 for Cronbach’s alpha. Of the 28 questions used in the original instrument seven were adapted for the current study with an alpha of .89. Personal Responsibility The four-item Personal Responsibility scale was constructed from one two-item scale and two additional questions. The two-item scale is derived from the Ascription of Responsibility factor within Schwartz’s Norm Activation Scale as reported by Guagnano, (1995). The items were originally identified by face validity and checked by conducting a factor analysis using principal-components techniques with oblimin rotation. Item factor loadings were both greater than .75 with an eigenvalue of 1.07 and Cronbach’s alpha of .65. The “I am partially responsible for the degraded state of our local rivers and streams” question was used in the Hwang et al. (2000) study and found to correlate with other variables in their study (which were similar the ones used in this research). The final item was constructed by the researcher and appears to have face validity. Therefore, three of the four items had either concurrent validity or construct validity as well as face validity. Internal consistency of personal responsibility scale used in this research was .81. 73 Intention to Act There were seven Intention to Act statements. Their design was based on information gathered from the scientific panel (see above) and a review of the literature. Each statement asked the respondent to indicate their level of support for each of the following items: zoning of open space; subsidies to landowners for environmentally friendly practices; stricter enforcement of current regulations; creation of new regulations; public information and education programs; and voluntary programs. Although the seven items were created to be independent of each other it was found they had an alpha of .88 when scaled. Questionnaire Construction and Design The resulting thirty multiple part questions were pre-tested with two separate groups of individuals. The first group of twenty individuals was drawn by reaching out to the local community and requesting volunteers. Many of the names were provided by MSU—Extension and Communications Department. This group was not considered to be representative of the Sycamore Creek watershed, but rather was a convenience sample necessitated by the availability of recording facilities. Each respondent was debriefed for twenty to thirty minutes after they had completed the survey. Questions were further revised based upon an item analysis of the pretest and comments made by the respondents. Revisions at this point were mostly minor, consisting of some additional wording and layout changes. The major change was the deletion of two questions on the creation of markets for pollution trading. Most respondents were not familiar enough with the concept to make a judgment and the researcher felt it was too difficult a concept for the survey to try and convey in addition to the current content. 74 The revised questionnaire was then pre-tested a second time. This time an intercept approach was taken to selecting respondents. People entering the Michigan Secretary of State office in Mason seeking their automobile license renewal were asked to participate while they waited. This was intended to be a convenience sample and was not representative of the watershed population. The main purpose of this pre-test round was to assess face and content validity aswell as user understandability. Ten individuals agreed to take the survey and be debriefed afierwards. Very minor changes to wording and layout were made based upon the respondent’s comments. The complete instrument consisted of thirty-four Likert scale questions (five choices from strongly disagree to strongly agree) on attitudes, personal responsibility and locus of control; twenty questions on the frequency of current uses and the importance of firture uses; one question on perceived water quality; five items on water quality knowledge and six on knowledge of best management practices; three choice experiment questions; seven on implementation information, one on preferred sources of information; and eight demographic questions. The number and complexity of the questions were limited by the time required to take the survey. It was felt that the questionnaire needed to be able to be completed within a twenty to thirty minute time frame (Dilhnan, 2000). As mentioned previously, the study roughly followed Dillrnan’s protocol for mail survey administration (Dilhnan, 2000). Five mailings were used to administer the survey. Seventeen hundred and fifiy randomly selected peOple were initially mailed a pre-letter introducing the study and informing them they would be receiving a questionnaire in the mail within the next week. The undeliverable pre-letters were deleted from the mailing 75 list for the first mailing of the survey and the list was further reduced to arrive at a total of 1,650 potential respondents. The questionnaire was mailed to potential respondents together with a postage-paid return envelope and a cover letter again explaining the study and reminding them that participation in the study indicated their informed consent. Reminder post cards were sent 14 days later to individuals who had not yet returned completed questionnaires. Twenty-five days after the first mailing, new cover letters and replacement surveys were sent to those whose original questionnaires were still outstanding. A final reminder post card was mailed 10 days later to individuals who had not yet returned a survey. Data Analysis There were 608 usable questionnaires returned plus 36 undeliverable, 23 return to sender and 19 blank surveys. Response bias was determined by telephoning 20 non- respondents to establish why they chose not to participate. To help the reader keep track of the research questions, variables and the items on the questionnaire they are summarized in Table 5. The data were analyzed using the statistical package, SPSS 10.1 (2001). Questions 3b and 22g were reverse coded so they were directionally consistent with each of the other variable questions. Then the FREQUENCIES procedure was applied to obtain a “picture” of the data in the form of raw frequencies and percentage of occurrence for the sample. Initially, descriptive statistics were referenced. Then, an exploratory factor analysis using principal axis analysis with varimax rotation was computed to determine how many attitudinal dimensions existed. Five factors with eigenvalues greater than 1.0 76 Table 5: Research @estions, Study Variables and the Items on the Questionnaire Variable Name Research Question Item(s) on Survey Attitude Q1: What attitudinal groups exist in the Q22a-t (independent) watershed? Q2: What is the composition of each group? Q22a—t: Q23-Q30 Q3: What are the fairness evaluations? Q22a-t Personal Q4: What is the relationship b/w attitude groups Q22a-t; Q3a-d Responsibility and Personal Responsibility? Knowledge of Q5: What is the relationship b/w attitude groups Q6-Q10 Issues and Knowledge of Issues? Knowledge of Q6: What is the relationship b/w attitude groups Q1 1-Q16 Action and Knowledge of Action Strategies? Strategies Locus of Q7: What is the relationship b/w attitude groups Q4a-f Control and PR? Intent to Act Q8: Do attitudinal groups differ in their Intent Q22a-t; Q20a-g (dependent) to Act? Q9: What is the relationship among all the All above items variables? Q10: Do the attitudinal groups differ in terms of All above items variables that influence Intention to Act? were produced. After dropping items that failed to load at greater than .40 on any of the five factors along with those items not meeting face validity inspection, there remained three factors. Confirmatory factor analysis was performed on the remaining three factors to establish internal consistency and to confirm the results of the principal axis analysis. Based on this analysis, the three factor solution was reduced to two. Confirmatory factor analysis was again run on the final two factors. Next, a hierarchical cluster analysis was performed on the resulting factors to identify groups of individuals who responded similarly to the attitude dimensions. An agglomeration schedule explaining the greatest difference between clusters was computed using a squared Euclidean distance measure of Ward’s method. The resulting dendrogram indicated there were two clusters Of respondents. K-means cluster analysis with two clusters (based on the hierarchical 77 results), was run to produce a new variable indicating respondent’s cluster membership. Finally, because the investigator was interested in the relationship between the resulting clusters of “types of attitudinal orientations” and socio-demographic variables the crosstabs procedure was run. The researcher was also interested in the relationships between the other study variables and the attitudinal orientations together with their influence on intention to act. After either a scale or index was created through summing individual responses to each item in a variable (see previous section on each variable), individual scores were assigned to the respondents and a new variable created. In order to better understand the relationships between all the variables, a correlation matrix was produced. Next, t-tests were computed to better understand the differences between clusters and each study variable. Finally, multiple regression (OLS) techniques were employed to evaluate the independent effects of specific variables on individual assessments of how to implement best management practices in the Sycamore Creek watershed. The regression analysis calculates results for the entire sample as well as for each cluster group. 78 CHAPTER IV: DATA ANALYSIS AND INTERPRETATION The purpose of this study was to assess the relationships between the antecedents of knowledge, attitude, locus of control and sense of responsibility on the intention to support the implementation of watershed best management practices. It was also designed to examine the fairness judgments underlying individual attitudes. This chapter has been divided into the following sections: (a) sample profile and biases, (b) constructing the attitudinal clusters, (c) cluster demographics, (d) fairness evaluations, (e) relationship between knowledge, personal responsibility, locus of control and the individualistic and egalitarian clusters, (1) the individualist and egalitarian clusters and the intention to act, (g) correlations between the study variables, (h) variables having the greatest influence on the intention to act, (1) non response survey and (j) study limitations. Sample Profile and Biases The following demographic variables were included in the study: age, income, education, number of people per household and length of residency in their current home and in the area. The purpose of profiling respondents was to address the representativeness of the sample to the Ingham County population and to see if demographic variables influence the intention to act. Population statistics were obtained from the 1990 and 2000 Census collected by the US. Census Bureau. Biases are addressed in the narrative. The results are in Table 5. 79 Table 6: Demographic Characteristics of Respondents Compared to the Population of Ingham County Demographic Sample Population Characteristics % % Age < 18 yrs old 26.41 (1) 18-24 years 1.0 8.4“ (2) 25-34 years 13.6 11.0.' (3) 35-44 years 20.4 17.1‘ (4) 45-54 years 26.4 18.41 (5) 55-64 years 18.4 10.0‘ (6) 65+ years 20.2 8.71 Education (1)0_ 00000588 mo. 05 00 805000 0:0 805 8080.50 5008588 0.3 80820 .. L _ 8 N. 20 a E a 5 2 8 _ _ a: ..w 08 a S 2 m8 2 _m 0 cm a wow 1 8 _ _ 80 w 0.8 .0 8 8 N8 5 :8 2 0.8 a E: 0 4. a 5 c8 4 0.8 m 0.8 a. .8 m S 4 08 m 8 m 8 m 8 ..m a N 8 N m. a ..m 03 _ 8 _ 8 08:8 3 0:85. .x. 8358 .x. #5 83 88 N .8830 n .8820 0.95% .308 8508 803.58 8000008 8 .: 805 005300 00 2:08 883.0003 800. .80 580: 8808:0880 8.8800— 05. 0>0n 08000 8 80.03 3 Emt 088 05 mm: 80800880 05. 800882 80503 880008 80.5 80008 8308 803000 05 «0 can 00 50: 2008 80802038 0555 088:3 8083 9880500 80% 008600 8 80000 2008 0800a 85 a 80008 8308808000 05 .3 800888 80003 8500008 000 a: 88020 8 00202: 00 9 0000 50: 8000 80850.38 0:... 30: 30:08 mac—08 005 85.508 0.08 8 0885 05 80.0 803.5003 w8>0m 50000 0:58 5830 05 80.5 008808 00 2:08 >05 0850805 000 88008 08 80>: 830 080b0>m 80080300 805 .«0 808058 05 .050 x00— 3 mam—£80080“ .808 0 08: 800.8% 89:. 880808 00888 8083 0>500b0 038 00 w80w 08 03 .5 8005808 580808 088 0508 8 038 >08 000b0>m 80808808 80003 w8£0>8 808$ 00 m8 805 8 Ewt 0 038 8858800 05 m0 800808 =< 800 00500508 000 a: 000—0 m0 088 805008 05 38 2:08 808 05 08:00 0.3 805. 02? 8:00 8 :05 ..050 020> 8: 80503 80 880 080 88 680 80 080 880 £8 £3 680 .80 808085 03880: 808.8”— 8080§m 0.885: 8088,,— nomm 80m 008080.505 0880M 0:0 53 0008< 055 88000080“ m0 0380803” “2 050... 96 considerations, (3) distributional justice consideration, (4) environmental and human rights and (5) governmental involvement. The number one ranked item by all respondents was “water has value other than its dollar value”. Other items that could be included in how respondents valued their waterways are Q22e, Q22r and Q22i. These items were ranked second, seventh and twelfth respectively. Taken as a group, these items focus on the trade off between the environment and the economy and how programs should be funded. There was strong support at the sample level for the polluter pays principle. Procedural justice considerations appeared third in the ranking by the entire sample. The statement “all members have a right to their say on issues involving water management” was agreed with by 88% of all respondents. Other items relating to procedural justice were Q22n and Q22g, ranked ninth and tenth respectively. These items as a whole spoke to the right to have voice, public involvement and a fair process. The third theme to emerge concerned itself with distributional justice considerations. There were two items relating to this theme: (Q22b) “everyone may have to make some personal sacrifices if we are going to have effective water resource programs” and (Q22d) “everyone owns rivers and streams and therefore they should be managed for the overall public benefit”. The former speaks to the distribution of costs, while the latter speaks to the distribution of benefits. Question 22m on the moral responsibility to look after your neighbors downstream (Q22m) (ranked as the fifth item by the entire sample), and the “environment having the same right to water as pe0p1e have” (Q220, constitutes a rights based theme. 97 Question 22m reflects human rights while Q22f concerned itself with environmental rights. The last theme to be considered important by all respondents was governmental involvement (Q22k). It was ranked eighth by all respondents. There were five of the twelve fairness heuristic items that appeared in the Australian water allocation study "top eight" rankings (Syme et al., 1999). At the universal fairness level, the Australian studies had ongoing support for the community’s rights to have a voice in the allocation decisions, the rights of the environment and having appropriate outcomes of procedural justice (Syme et al., 1999). Some of the similarities include an emphasis on the environment’s right to water with two of the Australian studies having about 78% - 81% of respondents supporting this notion, a similar result to this study’s findings. Another similarity between the studies was the weight placed on accepting the decisions made by a fair process. In the Australian studies support for these concepts ranged from 64% - 83%, while this study found 80% - 81% support. In terms of the differences between the two countries, those items contained in both studies that specifically traded off economic prosperity with environmental health appeared in this study’s ranking, but not in the Australian standings. Comparison Between Individualist and Egalitarian Cluster’s Fairness Evaluations There are statistically significant differences between how the two clusters of respondents, individualists and egalitarian, ranked these fairness heuristic items. The most glaring difference between the groups is their views on governmental involvement. While the egalitarian cluster ranked this item (Q22K) number one, the individualist cluster ranked it eleventh. This finding is consistent with the individualist view that 98 deregulation is a rational management strategy because they view nature as being benign. Also, individualists have a low group orientation and believe value decisions stem from personal judgments rather than collective control (Rayner, 19883) as might be represented by governmental involvement. Conversely, egalitarians might see governmental involvement as being able to help foster the equality of outcomes (Rayner, 1988b). It is also consistent with their high group philosophy (i.e. everyone should be involved). The differences between the cluster rankings on other fairness heuristic items would seem to support these findings. For instance, there were statistically significant differences between clusters on the items in the value of waterways theme. Most telling were items Q22r and Q22i that directly pitted environmental protection of our waterways against economic profit. In both these questions, the individualists were considerably less willing to forgo economic gain for the sake of a cleaner environment. Specifically, on the “saving waterways for the future being more important than making money” item only 73.2% were in agreement. And on the “long-term health of local waterways should be achieved even if it reduces short-term profits” item only 65.1% were in agreement. The egalitarians were 89% and 84% in agreement with these items respectively. The two clusters also differed on their level of agreement about “water having a value other than its dollar value” (Q22j) and the polluter pays principle (Q22e) but considerably less than the items just mentioned. Despite these apparent differences, two-thirds to three-quarters of the sample population are in agreement about these fairness heuristic concepts. The two clusters also differ significantly (t = -4.638, p = .000) on whether public involvement should be part of the decision making process. The individualist cluster does 99 not agree as strongly (73%) as the egalitarian group (87.3%) that the public should be involved. This is particularly interesting since both clusters do not differ (statistically) on “people’s right to have their say” (Q22a) and “accepting the decision of a fair process” (Q22n). The item on everyone having to make “personal sacrifices in order to have effective water resource program” (Q22b) was less supported by the individualist cluster (82%) than the egalitarian cluster (92%). This is consistent with individualists being less willing to forgo income to protect their waterways as discussed earlier. The last item on which the two clusters differ is on the “environments right to water being the same as people’s rights”. Seventy-three percent of the individualists agreed with this statement versus 87% by egalitarians. In summary, the two clusters were in agreement about water having a value other than its dollar value and the polluter pays principle. The items that they differed on supported our initial labeling of the clusters as individualist and egalitarian. That is, the individualist responses to the fairness heuristic items clearly demonstrated they favored less government involvement, were less willing to forgo economic gains for a cleaner environment, believed in the public's right to voice their Opinions but were less willing to be involved in decision making and placed people’s rights over that of the environment relative to their egalitarian counterparts. On the other hand, the egalitarian cluster was in favor of government involvement, were more willing to forgo economic gain for a cleaner environment, believed fair decision making processes involved the public and their right to voice themselves, and awarded the environment a higher level of rights, all relative to the individualist cluster. More importantly, despite the differences in the 100 degree of support indicated by the respective clusters, more than two-thirds to three- quarters of the sample population was in agreement with the concepts presented by each of the fairness heuristic items. These similarities can provide the basis for designing fiJture deliberative planning processes. Relationships Between Knowledge, Locus of Control and Personal Responsibility and the Individualist and Egalitarian Clusters If one thinks about the variables of knowledge, locus of control and personal responsibility in the context of the individualist and egalitarian clusters, one might be able to deduce what relationships might exist. For example, it would not be surprising if the data revealed that the individualist cluster was higher on locus of control than their egalitarian counterparts. This is because individualists value decisions stemming from personal judgments (Rayner, 1988a) and defend their freedom to bid and bargain in self- regulated networks (Thompson, 1992). In other words, they desire to be in control of their surroundings. Regarding personal responsibility, the egalitarian cluster is probably more likely to assume more personal responsibility for the environment than the individualist cluster. There are two reasons for this assertion: (1) egalitarians are thought to frame natural resource issues in ethical terms because it allows them to focus on the social and political dimensions and to criticize the institutions responsible for resource management (i.e. an “if I don’t take responsibility who will” attitude) (Dake, 1992) and (2) because individualists view nature as being benign, therefore not needing their attention. In terms of knowledge, there is little in the literature to guide us as to how this variable may relate to the two clusters. It could be argued that if egalitarians have more personal responsibility, then they are more likely to have more knowledge because the 101 latter is thought to be a pre-requisite for the former (Hines et al., 1986). Also, the previous demographic results indicating that the egalitarian cluster was more educated than the individualist cluster suggest this might be the case. Table 14 reports the differences between cluster means for the four psychological study variables. Table 14: Comparison of Study Variables by Attitudinal Groups Total In di vidualis ts Egalitarian t-test for Equality of Responses Means Std. Std. Std Sig. Mean Dev. Mean Dev. Mean Dev. t—test df (2-tailed) KNOWISS 2.56 1.45 2.50 1.46 2.66 1.44 ~1.399 597 .162 1 = no knowledge of issues 5 = knowledgeable of issues KNOWACT 3.07 1.59 2.92 1.61 3.28 1.54 -2.806 582 .005* CI' 2.33 -— 2.66CI‘ 2.50 — 2.83 1 = no knowledge of actions 5 = knowledgeable of actions LOC 3.64 .593 3.57 0.64 3.73 0.51 -3.367 597 .001* CI' 2.73—3.10CI‘ 3.10—3.47 1 = low perceived internal LOC 5 = high perceived internal Cl' 3.50 — 3.64 CI‘ 3.67 — 3.79 LOC PERSONAL 3.75 .597 3.63 0.60 3.88 0.57 -5.257 597 .000* 1 = low personal responsibility CI‘ 5 = high personal responsibility * Clusters are statistically different from one another at the .05 significance level 1 95% confidence interval 3.56 — 3.69 CI' 3.8] — 3.95 Environmental Knowledge The mean score for knowledge of the issue (KNOWISS) was 2.56 (SD = 1.45) with a minimum of one and a maximum of five. The mean score for the individualist cluster was 2.50 (SD = 1.46) and for the egalitarian cluster it was 2.66 (SD = 1.44). An independent groups t-test revealed there was no difference between the two cluster 102 groups with regard to their knowledge of the issues. The overlap between the clusters’ 95% confidence intervals confirms that there is little difference between the groups. The mean score for knowledge of action strategies (KNOWACT) was 3.07 (SD = 1.59) also with a minimum of one and maximum of five. The individualist’s mean score was 2.92 (SD = 1.61) while the egalitarian’s mean score was 3.28 (SD = 1.54). The individual group t-test indicated there was a statistically significant difference between the two clusters with the egalitarian group having more knowledge of action strategies. Inspection of the 95% confidence intervals shows there is no overlap between the two clusters and it can therefore be concluded the two groups do differ on this variable. Locus of Control The mean score on the locus of control (LOC) variable was 3.74 (SD = .89) ranging from a low of one to a high of five. The individualist cluster’s mean was 3.60 (SD = 0.67) while the egalitarian cluster’s score was 3.83 (SD = 0.81). The t-test for differences between groups indicated there was a difference and that the egalitarian cluster had a higher sense of locus of control than did their individualist counter parts. There was not a common range between the two confidence intervals. This is contrary to the anticipated results discussed previously, but lends support to Eden’s (1993) view that a person’s sense of environmental responsibility and efficacy are interdependent on how they influence pro-environmental behavior. Personal Responsibility The mean personal responsibility (PERSONAL) score was 3.91 (SD = 1.02) with a minimum of one and a maximum of five. Again, we see the egalitarian cluster as having 103 a larger mean score of 3.99 (SD = 0.78) than the individualist cluster’s mean of 3.75 (SD = 0.96). The individual group t-test indicated these to be two distinct groups as was shown by the separate confidence interval ranges. The result that the egalitarian cluster was higher on this variable was as anticipated and helps explain the LOC results. Summary The analysis of the data indicated that the egalitarian cluster was more knowledgeable about action strategies, believed it was in their power to make a difference (higher internal LOC), and assumed more personal responsibility for the management of local waterways relative to their individualist counterparts. Therefore, egalitarians were higher on all the psychological variables. This was almost what had been predicted, but originally it was thought that the individualist cluster would be higher on LOC, than the egalitarian cluster. A possible explanation for this finding is Eden’s (1993) observation that it seems meaningless to ascribe oneself responsibility for something one has no control over. The Individualist and Egalitarian Clusters and the Intention to Act Analysis of the data so far supports there being two distinct clusters of individuals in the study population with regard to their worldviews surrounding the management of the Sycamore Creek watershed. One significant question that remains to be answered is if these differing worldviews result in different intentions towards implementing best management practices. Table 15 presents how the study sample population and each of the clusters ranked the seven best management practices. 104 Table 15: Percentage Of Respondents Who Support and Ranked Preference for Each Of the Best Management Practice Options Total Sample Individualists Egalitarians (N=608) (N=320) (N2279) % Ranking % Ranking % Ranking Q20d Fines for polluting 86.6 1 82.2 1 94.3 1* Q20e Increased enforcement of environmental regulations Q20f Public information and education programs Q20g Voluntary programs to help landowners adOpt * environmentally friendly 80'] 3 '5 79-4 7- 83-8 5 practices QZOO Stricter regulations on activities that impact waterways during development Q20a Zoning requirements for some open space to be preserved on undeveloped land Q20b Subsidies to landowners for environmentally fiiendly 54.9 6 50.6 7 61.6 7* practices * Significant difference between means Of the cluster at the .01 level. 80.9 2 72.9 5 92.8 2* 80.1 3.5 76.9 3 86.4 4* 78.7 4 73.1 4 87.8 3* 71.0 5 64.3 6 81.0 6* When an independent group t—test was run between the cluster groups on the INTENT] variable (i.e. the sum Of all the BMP scores) there was a significant difference (t = -6.820, p = .000). In order to further analyze the BMP options, the percentage support for each option (would support and strongly support) was calculated and then each was ranked against one another. As a group, the respondents ranked fines for polluting as the number one BMP Option, followed by increased enforcement Of regulations. There was a tie for the third Option between public information and education and voluntary programs. Stricter regulations were fourth, followed by the zoning of Open spaces and lastly subsidies. The 105 fact that fines for polluting was ranked first by both clusters again reflect the polluter pays principle so prevalent in the results Of the analysis of the fairness heuristic. The two clusters began to diverge after this initial agreement in terms Of the types of programs they favored. The individualist cluster ranked both voluntary and public information programs as their second and third options respectively. They also gave lower rankings to increased enforcement (fifth) and stricter regulations (fourth). The egalitarian cluster was consistent in their desire for fines, increased enforcement and stricter regulation, ranking them first, second and third respectively. Neither cluster indicated a preference for the zoning of Open space nor subsidies and they were consistently ranked last. Subsidies were the most negatively received Option with 8.7 % Of the respondents indicated they would not support this option. The individualist cluster consistently had a lower percentage Of respondents who would indicate their support on all the items. All the differences in the rankings by the clusters were found to be statistically different by independent group t-tests. The “DO Not Know” category never exceeded 10% Of the responses both for the entire sample population and by cluster. Again despite the differences between the groups, five Of the six BMP Options received between two-third to three-quarters of the sample population’s support for implementation. Correlations Between Study Variables The seven study variables analyzed in this section are knowledge of the issues (KNOWISS), knowledge Of action strategies (KNOWACT), the attitudes of moral imperatives (MORAL), and resistance to change (RESISTI) that should be negatively correlated with the other variables, locus Of control (LOC), personal responsibility 106 (PERSONAL) and intention to act (INTENTl). Pearson Correlation analysis was conducted to explore the relationships among the variables (Table 16). Table 16: Pearson Correlations (N = 608) Between Study Variables (1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) (7) (1) KNOWISS 1 (2) KNOWACT .394** 1 (3) MORAL .146 .132" 1 (4) RESISTl -.076 -.079 -.127" 1 (5) LOC .111" .058 .370** -.090* 1 (6) PERSONAL .158M .134** .383" -.189** .460" 1 (7) INTENT] .091* .066 .414M -.260** .449" .364" 1 ** Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed) * Correlation is significant at the 0.05 level (2-tailed) The intention to support BMP implementation scores correlated significantly (p < .01) with (in order Of importance) LOC, MORAL, PERSONAL and RESISTl. Intuitively one would think that there would be a significant relationship between having knowledge and the intention to act, yet surprisingly there was not at the .01 level. Several other significant relationships were also found. KNOWISS was found to correlate with KNOWACT, MORAL, LOC and PERSONAL. Relationships were found between KNOWACT and the MORAL and PERSONAL variables. MORAL was found to correlate negatively with RESISTl and positively with LOC and PERSONAL. The RESIST] attitudinal measure exhibited a negative relationship with PERSONAL. And lastly, LOC was positively correlated with PERSONAL. The magnitudes Of the relationships are generally consistent with those studies reported in Table 4 at the end Of Chapter II. Nevertheless when certain variables were controlled for, many Of these relationships were no longer significant. Table 17 presents the partial correlation coefficients, significance levels and the controlled for variables. It can be seen amongst the antecedent variables to intention to act, the strongest relationships were between KNOWISS and KNOWACT, MORAL and 107 Table 17: Partial Correlations Coefficients Variables Coefficients Variables Controlled For KNOWISS - KNOWACT 374 p Z 000 MORAL, RESIST], LOC, ' ' PERSONAL KNOWISS - MORAL 034 Z 423 KNOWACT,RESIST1, LOC, ° 1’ ° PERSONAL KNOWISS — LOC 037 p Z 379 KNOWACT, MORAL, RESISTl, ' ' PERSONAL KNOWISS — PERSONAL 062 _ KNOWACT, MORAL, RESISTI, . p -— .141 LOC KNOWISS — RESIST] _ 017 Z 690 KNOWISS, MORAL, LOC, ' p ' PERSONAL KNOWACT - MORAL 049 p Z 238 KNOWISS, RESISTI, LOC, ' ' PERSONAL KNOWACT - RESIST] _ 054 Z 203 KNOWISS, MORAL, LOC, ' p ' PERSONAL KNOWACT — LOC _ 026 p Z 540 KNOWISS, MORAL, RESIST], ' ' PERSONAL KNOWACT — 067 p Z 112 KNOWISS, MORAL, RESIST], PERSONAL ° ' LOC MORAL - RESISTl _ 049 Z 240 KNOWISS, KNOWACT, LOC, ‘ P ° PERSONAL MORAL — LOC 231 Z 000 KNOWISS, KNOWACT, RESISTl, ' p ' PERSONAL MORAL - PERSONAL __ KNOWISS, KNOWACT, RESIST], .233 p - .000 LOC RESIST] — PERSONAL _ KNOWISS, KNOWACT, MORAL, -.149 p — .000 LOC RESIST] - LOC 088 Z 037 KNOWISS, KNOWACT, MORAL, ' p ' PERSONAL LOC — PERSONAL 367 Z 000 KNOWISS, KNOWACT, MORAL, . ' p ' RESIST] KNOWISS — INTENTI 010 Z 818 KNOWACT, MORAL, RESISTI, ' p ' LOC, PERSONAL KNOWACT — INTENTl _ 007 Z 867 KNOWISS, MORAL, RESISTI, ' p ' LOC, PERSONAL MORAL - INTENTI 268 _ 000 KNOWISS, KNOWACT, RESIST], ' p "‘ LOC, PERSONAL RESISTI - INTENT] _ 214 _ 000 KNOWISS, KNOWACT, MORAL, ‘ p " LOC, PERSONAL LOC -— INTENTI 297 __ 000 KNOWISS, KNOWACT, MORAL, ' P "' RESISTl, PERSONAL PERSONAL -— INTENT] 120 _ 004 KNOWISS, KNOWACT, MORAL, ° P ‘° RESIST], LOC 108 LOC, MORAL and PERSONAL, RESIST] and PERSONAL, RESIST] and LOC, and between LOC and PERSONAL. Regarding the intention to act variable (INTENTl), it can be seen that MORAL, RESIST], LOC and PERSONAL were all significant. The difference between the partial correlation that controlled for all other study variables and the Pearson Correlation on the relationship between KNOWISS and KNOWACT was only .022. This insignificant difference between the results suggests that these two measures of knowledge may not be as distinct as had originally been intended. Therefore, these two measures will be summed together to create one variable for use in the forth-coming regression analysis. The attitudinal variables of MORAL and RESIST] along with the other psychological variables Of LOC and PERSONAL interacted as was anticipated through a review of the literature. Specifically, the following can be said about these relationships: (1) the relationship between the two attitudinal variables of MORAL and RESIST] were not significant in the partial correlation, (2) three Of the correlations, MORAL - LOC, MORAL - PERSONAL and RESIST] - PERSONAL were all substantially lower in the partial correlation analysis and (3) the LOC — PERSONAL relationship remained relatively strong in the partial correlation analysis. These Observations would seem to indicate a high level Of interrelatedness between the psychological variables used in the study. The MORAL attitudinal factor was clearly viewed by respondents as being similar to both the LOC and PERSONAL (responsibility) scales. The most robust relationship was between locus of control and personal responsibility (.367, p = .000). When each of the variables was correlated with the overall intention to support best management practices while controlling for all the other variables, it was again 109 found that the relationships were weaker than in the Pearson Correlation. The variable most resistant to change in the partial correlation was RESIST] while the most significant variable to change was PERSONAL. Variables Having the Greatest Influence on the Intention to Act Multiple regression (OLS) analysis was used to assess the independent effects Of specific variables on individual intention to support BMPS designed to improve water quality. For the independent variable assessing cluster membership, a dummy variable was constructed for use in the regression analysis. Table 18 presents the mean and standard deviation Of each variable, their Beta weights, t-tests and Significance levels. The F- test results indicate the model is statistically significant. For the various demographic variables included in the model, it was found that people with higher levels Of education were more likely to support the implementation Of BMPS. The remaining variables Of age, income, length Of residency in the area and in their current home were not found to be significant. The knowledge of issues and action strategies variables were also not significant. This is consistent with other studies that have found that knowledge has a weak relationship with either attitudes or intentions (McFarlane & Boxall, 2000). The next sets Of variables included in the model were the psychological measures. This included the GROUP variable that encompassed the two attitudinal factors of moral imperatives and resistance tO change and the other psychological variables Of locus Of control and personal responsibility. All three of the variables had the expected effect on the intention to support BMP implementation and were statistically significant. Memberships in the egalitarian cluster 110 Table 18: Multiple Regression Estimates for the Intention tO Support BMP KNOWALL LOC PERSONAL GROUP AREA HOME AGE EDUC INCOME INTENT Implementation Knowledge Of Issues & Action Strategies 1 - no knowledge 5 -— loiowledgeable Locus of Control 1 - low internal control 5 —— high internal control Personal Responsibility 1 - low sense of personal responsibility 5 -— high sense Of personal responsibility Cluster Membership 0 - individualists ] —— egalitarians Length Of Residency in Area continuous Length Of Residency in Current House continuous Age Of Respondent l -— 18 - 24 4 - 45 - 54 6 - 65 years Of age and up Education Level 1 — less than high school 3 — some college 5 — post graduate degree Household Income 1 — less than $10,000/year 6 - $40,000 -— $49,000/year 9 -— $70,000t/year Intent to Support BMPS Options 1 — would not support 3 - would support 4 — would stroggly support Mean 2.78 3.66 3.77 .502 33.89 16.05 4.02 3.48 6.24 3.23 Std. Dev. .954 .589 .608 .500 18.96 13.93 1.32 1.05 2.05 .538 Beta -.009 .355 .179 .137 -.038 -.060 -.026 .094 .002 t-test -.227 8.310 4.070 3 .426 -.686 1.111 -.492 2.090 .059 Sig. Level .821 .000* .000* .001 * .493 .267 .623 .037* .953 R = .557 R2 = .310 Adjusted R2 = .297 * Significant at the .05 level F = 24.272 p = .000 would have a greater propensity towards supporting the implementation of BMPS than would being in the individualist cluster. The locus Of control indicator was found to be 111 the strongest predictor Of whether an individual will support BMPS or not. For the final indicator in this set of variables, personal responsibility, it was found that if a person felt responsible for the health of local waterways then they were more likely to support the implementation of BMP, although this was the weakest predictor of the significant variables. The model had an R of .557 and an adjusted R2 of .3 1. These figures, although below the range that Sutton (1998) reported in his meta-analysis, still provide support for the argument that environmental intention to act is primarily influenced by the psychological underpinnings of worldviews. The psychological indicators were the most important determinants of the intention to support implementing BMPS. Not only do all the indicators have significant effects on the intention to support in the model, but the largest standardized regression coefficients was locus of control (B = .355). When the sample was subdivided by the clusters and the regression model applied, there is only a small difference in the model’s predictability (Table 19). Specifically, the model better predicts the individualist cluster (R = .563) than it does the egalitarian cluster (R = .555) and with fewer variables. The models for both clusters were significant with the F — test for individualists being 14.3444 (p = .000) and for egalitarians it was 11.926 (p = .000). In order Of importance, the variables Of LOC, MORAL and RESIST are the significant predictors Of an individualist member's intention to support the implementation Of BMPS. For egalitarians the significant predictor variables, in order Of importance are LOC, MORAL, EDUC and PERSONAL. Note that there are differences in the list Of variables that are effective in predicting the intention to act for each cluster. 112 Table 19: By Cluster Regression Estimates for the Intention to Support BMP Implementation Individualist Egalitarian b Beta b Beta KNOWALL Knowledge OfIssues&Act1on -.039 -.067 -.009 -.019 Strategies 1 - no knowledge 5 — knowledgeable MORAL Moral Imperatives .352 .322a .259 .245m 0 — Low moral priority 5 — high moral priority RESIST] Resistance to Change -.]72 .068c .020 -.017 O —— low resistance to change 5 -— high resistance to change LOC Locus Of Control .290 .324“I .256 .272”| 1 — low internal control 5 — high internal control PERSONAL Personal Responsibility .001 .018 .104 .122c 1 - low sense of personal responsibility 5 - high sense Of personal responsibility AREA Length of Residency in Area -.000 —.026 .000 .000 continuous HOME Length Of Residency in Current House .000 .026 .002 -.057 continuous . AGE Age Of Respondent .000 -.127 -.025 -.067 l — 18 - 24 4 — 45 - 54 6 - 65 years of age and up EDUC Education Level .030 .042 .009 .198" 1 - less than high school 3 - some college 5 - post graduate degree INCOME Household Income .000 .003 .003 .014 1 — less than $10,000/year 6 - $40,000 — $49,000 9 - $70,000+/year R = .563 R = .555 R2=.3l7 R2=.3O8 Adjusted R2 = .295 R2 = .282 F: 14.344 F: 11.926 p = .000 p = .000 a: Difference Between the Clusters is Significant at the .001 level b: Difference Between the Clusters is Significant at the .0] level 0: Difference Between the Clusters is Significant at the .05 level There are also similarities in that the variables Of LOC and MORAL are common to both clusters. Both LOC and MORAL beta weights are stronger for individualists than for 113 egalitarians. These findings suggest that the clusters do indeed think differently about their watershed and by extension, how it should be managed. Non-Response Results TO examine the somewhat low response rate, twenty phone contacts were made to determine why the surveys were not completed and returned. The most common reason for non-response was that the individual was “too busy.” Four respondents were no longer at that mailing address. Three people felt they were too Old to participate. Two respondents indicated they did not know enough to participate and the remainder Of the pe0p1e did not respond for various other reasons. Not knowing enough about watershed issues to adequately answer the survey was also one of the reasons given for refiasal when trying tO recruit participants for pre-testing. Study Limitations Before advancing to the conclusions Of this study, there are several limitations that should be acknowledged. It is important to address these limitations to fully understand the conclusions and recommendations that have come about as a result of this research. First, the study chose to use the fairness heuristic in order to capture the interaction between respondent attitudes and fairness judgments. Although use of this instrument enriched the analysis in terms Of respondent worldviews, it weakened the subsequent analysis between attitudes and the other psychological study variables. That is, the attitudinal factors Obtained were less defined than one might like, although they 114 still met normal conventions. Also, there was some collinearity between the other study variables but this is Often the case in psychological studies. Second, personal responsibility was used as a proxy for the social norm variable in the theory Of planned behavior. Although personal responsibility is a social norm (Geller, 1995; Guagnano, 1995; Schwartz, 1970, 1977) it may be that it is two narrow a construct to adequately capture the nuances that Ajzen and Fishbein (1977) envisioned the construct to mean. Furthermore there was some collinearity with the other study variables Of moral imperatives and LOC. Another limitation Of the study was the ability to capture the situational factors surrounding the implementation Of best management practices. The details associated with BMPS such as size and location if it is a structural BMP or the type of payment vehicle for both structural and non-structural BMPS was not reflected in the questionnaire. This means that a respondent’s intention to support the implementation Of a BMP may change once they are in possession of all the facts. Resource managers need to be aware of the potential impact Of these details and not rely solely on the conclusions drawn by this study. Finally, the actual watershed used in the study, Sycamore Creek, has been the site for state extension programs since the late 1980s. The previous efforts to inform and educate the public may have influenced respondent knowledge, attitudes, locus Of control and their sense Of personal responsibility that in turn influenced their intention to support BMP implementation. Managers from other watersheds reading these conclusions and recommendations need to recognize that each watershed is unique and not simply assume it will be the same in their watershed. 115 CHAPTER V: CONCLUSIONS, DISCUSSION AND RECOMMENDATIONS Conclusions and Discussion There has been a limited amount of research using psychological analysis to predict future behavioral intentions towards watershed management programs. This study sought to take one psychological research instrument (the fairness heuristic) and one psychological theory (the theory Of planned behavior) and apply them in the context Of watershed best management practices. The study set out to determine groups of people who held similar worldviews on how to manage their watershed. Analysis Of respondents discovered two attitudinal factors, MORAL IMPERATIVES and RESISTANCE TO CHANGE. Using these two factors, respondents were clustered into two groups, individualists and egalitarian, borrowing from Dake’s (1991, 1992) worldview typology. The analysis further defined the characteristics of these groups in terms of demographics, fairness evaluations and psychological variables. When the groups were broken down by these variables and interpreted through the lens of grid/ group nomenclature, it was concluded that the worldviews represented by these names indeed fit the respondent groups. The last stage of the analysis sought to determine if the two groups of respondents differed in their support for implementing BMPS and what study variables best predicted their support. It was concluded that they did differ on both the level of their support and the types Of programs preferred. Also, the clusters used different study variables in making these determinations. 116 The use Of the fairness heuristic and the theory Of planned behavior were used tO examine the effects of psychological variables on the intention to support BMP implementation. The use Of these different instruments helped to reinforce the difficulty Of separating the procedural justice aspects Of decision-making from those associated with attitudes and outcomes (Folger, 1996; Syme et al., 1999). Still, the combination Of these approaches yielded many important findings. These discoveries are discussed below. Exploratory factor analysis Of the fairness heuristic yielded two attitudes held by respondents in terms Of managing their watershed. These were labeled MORAL IMPERATIVES and RESISTENACE TO CHANGE. The former factor seemed similar to the findings that people believe in the “rights Of the environment” found in the Australian water allocation studies (Syme et al., 1999). This bodes well for resource managers in that they can focus on those similarities when working with the community to formulate policy. Regarding the resistance to change factor, it seemed to indicate a desire for minimal government and the inalienability of property rights both Of which are tenants of libertarian doctrine (Wenz, 1988). Again, this might help resource managers when they are working with the community. If the pOpulation is in agreement about desired outcomes (i.e. clean water) then it is only the ways in which to achieve this that are contentious. Knowing there is a large portion of the population who will be resistant to certain types of change, the manager can then suggest Offering a suite Of implementation Options aimed at cleaning up waterways. Concerning the other items in the fairness heuristic, they were only modestly correlated which suggests that people think about these items somewhat independently. 117 The cluster analysis based upon these two attitudes showed two clusters existed in the study population. These were labeled individualist and egalitarian. The individualist and egalitarian groups Of respondents were relatively close in terms of their MORAL IMPERATIVES attitude. This is encouraging because it suggests that people generally agree on a common set Of ethical principles upon which to decide how to manage their watershed (Seligrnan et al., 1994). The two groups were not as close in their attitude Of RESISTANCE TO CHANGE, with the individualist group indicating they were more resistant than their egalitarian counter parts. If one assumes that part Of a person’s resistance to change is somewhat related to “fear Of the unknown”, then the implications Of this finding for the watershed manager is a need to anticipate the consequences Of proposed programs and communicate these effectively to watershed residents. Furthermore, it can be anticipated that a portion of the population will support programs that have the least impact on themselves, regardless of program effectiveness. In order to further understand and confirm the worldviews held by the two groups Of respondents, the study variables were analyzed at both the sample population level and at the cluster level. These yielded numerous interesting results beginning with the demographic profiles. Beginning at the sample population level, it was shown that the sample had obtained higher levels of education, earned slightly more money and had fewer children than did the general pOpulation Of homeowners in Ingham County. Based on previous research stating that young, educated liberals were more likely to possess pro-environmental attitudes, (Arcury & Johnson, 1987; Arcury et al., 1986; Christianson & Arcury, 1992) we might wish to conclude that selection bias is an issue for the study. 118 Fortunately, subsequent analysis Of the groups revealed substantial differences in responses suggesting that this effect was minimal. The individualist cluster was found to be Older, less educated, have a lower household income, fewer children and resided in the area and their home longer. Relative to these findings, it was shown that the egalitarian cluster was younger, more educated, had a higher household income and resided in the area and their home for shorter periods Of time. The multiple regression analysis indicated that of these variables, only the level of education for egalitarians was a significant predictor of the intent to support BMP implementation. There are two conclusions to be drawn from these findings on demographic variables: (1) the profiles Of each group may help a watershed manager identify which group an individual or group of individuals is likely to be associated with, but that (2) the results only partially supported previous research suggesting that younger, more educated, urban dwelling liberals are more likely to engage in pro-environmental behavior (Arcury & Johnson, 1987; Arcury et al., 1986; Christianson & Arcury, 1992). Therefore, demographic variables may only be useful in helping to identify individuals who possess similar worldviews and the type of interventions to use. Those fairness heuristic items in which there appeared to be the greatest variability in the population sample related to human use. There was a significant spread in thoughts about water and water pollution in an economic context. In general, most people were neutral on whether people should receive compensation for programs that hurt an individual’s livelihood, but were more variable about using programs to maximize the local economy. Furthermore, this variability carried over into using 119 cost/benefit analysis to solve water pollution problems and to trading Off different parts Of the environment for human use. The variability Of these items suggests that respondents were unsure about viewing water resources in economic terms and that there may be other considerations that entered their reasoning. The most highly skewed responses involved moral Obligations (to those downstream), community voice, non-economic value and distributive justice. The strong desire by respondents to adhere to these concepts when managing the watershed seems to indicate they held a rights based viewpoint, as is associated with a universal value orientation (Axelrod, 1994). This has immediate implications for water pollution trading programs. If the public has the perception Of clean water as a right, then they may not be receptive to economic programs, especially if the programs are not understood. At the very least, it can be anticipated that there will be conflict between environmental rights and other constitutional or legal rights (Montada & Kals, 2000). From this point forward in the analysis, the grid/ group nomenclature associated with Dake’s (1991,1992) worldviews was used as a lens for interpretation. Specifically, the social relations surrounding individualists are hypothesized to hold the myth Of nature as benign so that if pe0p1e are released from artificial constraints there will be few limits to abundance for all with surplus tO provide compensation for any hazards created in the process (Dake, 1992). Deregulation is the rational management strategy in this low- grid/low-group culture because individualists value decisions stemming from personal judgments rather than collective control (Rayner, 1988a). The term individual in this context refers tO social beings generating and stabilizing a form of social relations and institutions that defend their freedom to bid and bargain in self-regulated networks with 120 few prescriptions (Dake, 1992). On the other hand, egalitarian groups are those with strong group boundaries (high group), but with prescriptions that do not vary by rank and station (Dake, 1992). They believe the myth that nature is fragile and because they view nature as being ephemeral it justifies their precautionary approach to management. The egalitarian group prefers approaches to management that foster equity of outcomes (Rayner, 1988b) and are hypothesized to frame natural resource issues in ethical terms. When egalitarian social relations prevail, they are ofien critical Of the institutions responsible for natural resource management and in the extreme form can be strict preservationists (Dake, 1992). The data analysis findings did support that the two study clusters fit these social patterns and the findings will be subsequently discussed. It should be noted that measures of a worldview are related to personality traits and personal values as well as to social attitudes and policy preferences and as such are not meant to be mutually exclusive categories. Ranking the responses to the fairness heuristic at both the sample population and cluster levels was the next step in the analysis. Consistent with the highly skewed responses, the number one ranked item by the sample population was that “water has a value other than its dollar value.” The individualist cluster also ranked this item first while the egalitarian cluster ranked it second. Question 22f directly asked respondents to consider whether the environment has the same rights to water as people; it was ranked ninth by the egalitarian cluster and tenth by the individualist cluster. Therefore, the same conclusion that was already made about respondents awarding the environment at least some rights can be drawn. 121 Either the second or third ranked item by all respondents was the desire for polluter pay programs. This stated preference indicates the public Operates in a command and control management paradigm and wish to internalize the extemalities of pollution. The limitations Of command and control programs have been well documented (Montada & Kals, 2000; Tenbrunsel et al., 1997), but the desire by the public for these types of programs would seem to indicate a lack of comprehension about their limitations. Also, consistent with water having a value other than its dollar value, the desire for command and control programs can be interpreted as support for rights based viewpoints: people who pollute and effect others in the community should have to make restitution (Montada & Kals, 2000). The finding that some respondents were more concerned about forgoing economic gain for the sake of a cleaner environment than they were about process considerations is contrary to the Australian water allocation studies (Syme et al., 1999). In the Australian studies, it was found that fair decision-making processes were of paramount importance and that economic arguments were Of a lesser importance to process considerations (Syme et al., 1999). In this study, more than half the respondents (individualists, N = 320) thought economic arguments more important than process consideration. Furthermore, their moderate support for the long-term health of the environment if it reduces short-term profits (Q22i = 65.1%) indicated a desired to for polluter pay policies that have minimal immediate economic impact. These frndings are consistent with the individualist view that if artificial constraints (regulation and enforcement) are removed, there will be few limits to abundance for all and that this will more than compensate for any hazards created in the process (Rayner, 19883). Conversely, egalitarian support for 122 these items can be interpreted as being consistent with advocating prescriptions that do not vary by rank and station (Dake, 199]). The implications of this finding for watershed managers is that they need to be particularly aware Of program impacts and need to find innovative ways Of firnding their initiatives. The egalitarian and individualist clusters differ significantly on the procedural justice consideration of whether the public should be involved in decision-making. The individualist cluster does not agree as strongly (73%) as the egalitarian group (87 .3%) that the public should be involved. This is particularly interesting since both clusters dO not differ (statistically) on people’s right to have their say (Q22a) and accepting the decision of a fair process (Q22n). This would seem to indicate that there is a difference in what the two clusters consider a fair process. If individualists wish to have their say but not be as involved in the decision making process, then their criteria for a fair process probably includes less up front public involvement. Again, this is consistent with the individualist worldview: they seek to generate and stabilize a form of social relations and institutions that defend their freedom to bid and bargain in self-regulated networks with few prescriptions (Thompson, 1992). This low group/low grid finding would suggest that individualists would support the current American legal system where they can voice their concerns after a problem is perceived. The two groups also differed significantly on whether the government should be involved in decision-making processes with the individualist group indicating a greater desire for minimal governmental intervention. Therefore, watershed-planning initiatives should strive to be seen as non-governmental agencies and autonomous Of political affiliation. Furthermore, planning bodies need to recognize the necessity of soliciting 123 help from all facets Of the community, not just those willing to participate (i.e., most likely egalitarian groups). A trusted spokesperson that can voice individualist concerns and communicate the initiative’s intentions back to all factions of the community will go a long way towards having programs gain public acceptance. Having established the differences between the clusters based on the fairness heuristic responses, it is necessary to point out that more than two-thirds to three-quarters Of the sample population agreed with these statements. Watershed managers need to recognize that there are more similarities than differences between these groups. In most cases the differences that do exist are more a matter of degree and are concerned with how to proceed with implementation rather than being irreconcilable differences. As will be shown, this trend towards the presence of group differences within the context Of overall general agreement is a common element to most Of the study variables. Comparisons Of this study’s conclusions to those found by the Australian studies indicate some differences. Although both agreed that a large portion Of people believe in the “rights of the environment” and its preservation for a range of uses for future generations (Syme et al., 1999), they differed on their value Of process. That is, the Australian study put process above economic concerns while at least half of the respondents in this study felt the Opposite. Furthermore, not all eight items common to both studies and as ranked by the Australian studies appeared in this study’s rankings and those that did were ranked differently. These differences probably indicate there are significant cultural distinctions between the countries on how they view and manage the environment and in particular, water resource management. 124 A country’s laws reflect its culture and a brief look at the differences between the United States and Australian judicial systems may help explain the differences noted above. In the United States there is an “absolutist approach” with a heavy emphasis on the supremacy Of the law, particularly the Constitution, and an attempt to avoid substantive issues (Bossehnann, 1997). On the other hand, Australia adOpts a “balance of interests” approach that attempts to weigh all the various interests (Bosselrnann, 1997). Specifically, Ministerial discretion is awarded to environmental agencies on when and when not to institute certain procedures (i.e. environmental impact assessments) (Meyer, 1996). The public then has the right to appeal ministerial decisions. Consequently there is a lack Of judicial review and an emphasis on collaborative solutions in Australia (Meyer, 1996). Conversely, in the United States the importance of the law creates an atmosphere of scientific dominance where scientific results are used to support varying positions surrounding an issue and leaving the courts to sort out solutions. This adversarial system Of checks and balances pits individuals and institutions against one another and Offers a possible explanation as to why this study found process consideration ranked lower than economic considerations by half the respondents. There is an implicit belief that information causes pro-environmental behavior (Eden, 1993). This research supports previous studies (Beckwith & Ray], 2002; Hwang et al., 2000; Steel et al., 1990; Tarrant et al., 1997) that have found the impact Of policy- relevant knowledge on behavioral intent to have little bearing. Even though there was a statistical difference between the knowledge Of action strategies between the two clusters, it was not a determinant in predicting the intention tO support BMP implementation. The partial correlations between attitude, locus Of control and personal responsibility with 125 intent to act changed little when knowledge was controlled for. This means that knowledge is mediated, almost completely, by these variables. Interestingly, respondents cited a lack of knowledge as one reason for not wishing to participate in the study. If people are not making decisions based on what they know (or aware of), then other variables must be influencing their attitudes. There two possible explanations for this finding which more than likely are interdependent. First, the measures used tO indicate knowledge in this study could be characterized as awareness about both the issues and action strategies. Although awareness does constitute a form of knowledge, it is arguably at a surface level and therefore may not invoke cognitive interaction with other study variables by respondents. The second possible explanation for the weak relationship between knowledge and the other variables might be due to Steel et al.’s (1990) suggestion that Americans are highly influenced by ideological and environmental value orientations (Steel et al., 1990). That is, the values the American people assign to environmental issues and the management decisions they support may have more to do with political ideology than how they value the environment. The issue Of transmitting knowledge to the public about BMP implementation is an important one. It was shown earlier that there is a real need to educate the public about the limitations Of command and control regulations and the possibility of other types of programs. Yet, there exist two distinct ideological groups within the watershed each with their own worldview of the environment. This suggests that not only is the content Of the message provided to the different constituencies complicated and problematic, but so is the issue of how to get the groups to even examine the message in the first place. 126 The theory Of planned behavior was found tO provide quite an accurate prediction Of the intention to implement BMPS. In accordance with the theory, attitudes towards watershed management, subjective norms (personal responsibility) and perceptions Of behavior control were significant determinants of the intent to support the implementations Of BMPS. The successful application of the theory of planned behavior to the intention to support the implementation of BMPS is consistent with other research in which the theory effectively predicted intentions. The correlation for the sample pOpulation was .56 and for the individualist and egalitarian groups .56 and .55 respectively. These results are on the low end Of previous studies reported in Sutton’s (1998) meta-analysis. There are a few reasons why this might have occurred. The first reason for the low (but still significant) correlations could be due to the fact that attitudinal factors derived from the exploratory factor analysis were not as tightly defined as if they had been determined a priori. Second, the variable Of personal responsibility was used as a proxy for social norms. Although used in this way in the past (Geller, 1995; Guagnano, 1995; Schwartz, 1970, 1977), personal responsibility is only one aspect Of social norms and may be tOO closely associated with both locus Of control and the attitudes used in this study. Further evidence of these possible limitations comes from the reduced partial correlations and collinearity between the attitudinal factors (MORAL IMPERATIV ES and RESISTANCE TO CHANGE) and the locus of control (LOC) and personal responsibility variables. Having couched the results in this context, the finding that egalitarians had a higher internal LOC than individualists and that there was collinearity between the study variables is consistent with Eden’s (1993) supposition that feeling responsible is often dependent upon a belief in efficacy. That is, individuals 127 feel they can have some impact through their pro-environmental behavior as well as being able to choose what they undertake. According to Eden: “Where efficacy is not perceived, responsibility is weakened because, without impact, individual acts are futile. It becomes, for most, pointless to ascribe responsibility to the self to undertake behavior which has no effect.” (Eden, 1993, p. 1748) This link with impact rather than moral Obligation has a utilitarian ethos to it (Eden, 1993) since responsibility is not initiated because of morality, but because of perceiving the ability to limit environmental damage. This is important to this study because in the multiple regression analysis, personal responsibility was not a determining variable in predicting the individualist cluster’s intent to implement BMPS. Eden Offers this possible explanation: the perceived impact is significant and this supports Schwartz’s (1970, 1977) emphasis on the individual’s awareness of the consequences of his or her actions as contributing to responsibility ascription. Contexuality is therefore influential in that it affects how efficacy is perceived. Efficacy is reinforced for activists by their group situation, underlying the necessity of setting individual pro- environmental behaviors within social circumstances.” (Eden, 1993, p. 1749) Therefore, even though individualists saw it in their control to dO something about the problem Of degraded water quality, they were unlikely to act upon it (relative to the egalitarian cluster) because they did not feel as responsible. The relative lack of personal responsibility felt by the individualist cluster was probably a result of their cultural relations that reinforce their belief that the environment is benign and does not need their attention. The implication Of this insight for the watershed manager is that they must create and help maintain social organizations that are supported by all facets Of the community so as to instill a sense Of personal responsibility across all groups. 128 The other difference between the variables that helped predict the intent to implement BMPS was that education level was a determinant for egalitarians but not for individualists. One possibility for this result mentioned previously is that advanced levels of education are Ofien associated with more liberal worldviews (Arcury et al., 1986; Christianson & Arcury, 1992), a variable nOt measured in this study. Still, there is evidence that this could be true. Recall in the analysis of the clusters, the egalitarians that are assumed tO exhibit more liberal tendencies were also more likely to support all the BMP Options. These results lead to the conclusion that simply possessing knowledge (awareness) about the issues and the implementation Of BMPS is not sufficient to get people to support these types Of policy initiatives. Again it was shown that there were group differences, but that generally the study population was in agreement in terms of their psychological orientations towards the BMP implementation. The correlations between the study variables Of attitudes, LOC and personal responsibility were all positive and substantial and both the knowledge categories were not significant. SO even though there were statistical differences between the individualist and egalitarian clusters, their mean values for each Of the respective scales indicated they both had a positive moral imperative attitudes, a negative resistance to change attitudes, positive LOC and personal responsibility. There were some significant differences between individualists and egalitarians in terms of the types of BMP programs they preferred after their initial agreement on fines for polluters. Specifically, individualists sought voluntary and public information and education programs as their second and third choices and were less supportive Of all the programs than the egalitarian cluster. This finding is consistent with their low grid 129 orientation, their general desire for fewer prescriptions and being more resistant to change. The resistance to change attitude involved issues such as minimal governmental intervention (i.e. more freedom) and the property rights. Therefore, it would seem individualists exhibit integrative or “trade-Off" reasoning (Peterson, 1994) as is associated with a value pluralism approach (Sniderman et al., 1991; Sniderman & Tetlock, 1986b; Tetlock, 1986) to decision-making. Recall, this is where all their core values are highly and equally prized in the decision-making frame of reference. Egalitarians preferred increased enforcement and stricter regulations in terms Of their policy preferences and generally indicated greater support for all the policy categories. This conflicts with their low grid orientation but is consistent with their desire to create an equitable playing field within the community. One might expect these policy choices as they are seen as being generally valid and guarantee more equality for members of the community (Montada & Kals, 2000). The egalitarian’s frame for decision-making is more focused on the environment and therefore more monastic. As was seen previously, they were also more Open to procedural justice initiatives. These Observations suggest that an egalitarian’s frame of reference for decision-making involves both self-ascription and having an implicit assessment Of efficacy (Eden, 1993) compared to their individualist counterparts. Neither cluster indicated a preference for the zoning Of Open space or subsidies with these options consistently being ranked last. The strong economic values that came forth in the analysis are probably responsible for the negative evaluation of subsidies, while lack Of support for increased zoning restrictions is more than likely a result Of the strong support for property rights. Once more we see that despite the group differences, 130 there was relatively strong support for five of the seven proposed BMPS by all the respondents. In general, invoking the concept Of framing refers tO the perceptual lenses, worldviews or underlying assumptions that guide communal interpretation and definition Of particular values. Use Of the concept of framing reflects the growing acknowledgement that how societies view the environment is not simply given by nature, but reflects collective moral choices about the legitimacy of the myriad of intersections between natural and human systems (Bardwell, 199]; Miller, 2000). This research has demonstrated that the worldviews of the two clusters of respondents, individualists and egalitarians, influenced their assessments of environmental change including the terms Of participation, the range Of policy Options considered and the nature Of the political intervention. These Often divergent worldviews indicate there is an “interpretive flexibility” (Collins & Pinch, 1982) surrounding watershed management. It has already been shown that at the macro scale a country’s laws can serve to establish context and that previous efforts within the planning of Sycamore Creek watershed might have helped establish local context but neither of these by themselves or in cooperation can explain the subtle nuances surrounding the differences between the two cluster’s worldviews. One needs to better understand the social histories and dynamics of the area in order to achieve a deeper understanding of these groups. Obviously if there is general agreement about the desired outcomes, but not on how to achieve it, then a more in-depth look at how the groups frame the issues is needed so planning initiatives can move forward. It may be simply that individuals do draw on political ideology (Steel et al., 1990) to frame watershed issues but this may also not be the case. The Obvious implication of this 131 conclusion is that deliberation must occur in order to create policies that reflect local context and make them acceptable to the community. Implications for the Management of Sycamore Creek Watershed With the growing tendency for the public tO become more involved in decisions regarding the management of natural resources, there are practical implications to a greater understanding of the relationships among public values, attitudes and knowledge. First, attitudinal information can help managers understand the diverse sides Of watershed management issues. Increasingly, the management Of viable natural ecosystems can represent a multiplicity of public values (Bengston, 1994). Given that they must manage natural resources in the public interest, managers must recognize the extent tO which these values and value orientations drive public attitudes towards specific issues. In this study, it was shown there were two groups Of peOple with different value orientations or worldviews within the watershed and that the public is not simply monolithic in their views Of how to manage watershed even if there is mutually agreed upon goal. Second, a significant amount Of research in social psychology and natural resource management has supported the notion that attitudes predispose or predict behavior. Such behavior can take an active form, as in participating in decision-making processes, or a more passive form such as support for specific watershed management practices. This is important because many decisions regarding resource management are being brought forward tO the public through ballot initiatives. Regardless Of the outcome of these initiatives, it is apparent that watershed managers must understand the nature Of public attitudes and the resulting behavior. Using the identified cluster’s worldviews as a lens to anticipate public reaction to prOposed policies is one way for watershed managers 132 to understand public attitudes and behavior. For example, any proposed policy calling for immediate implementation that had an associated cost for the landowner attached tO it, and administered by a government agency, would probably be Opposed by the individualist cluster. Conversely, any proposed policy that did not have equality Of outcomes for all individuals would probably meet Opposition from the egalitarian cluster. Both of these reactions can be anticipated regardless Of whether the groups agreed upon and desired the same end result. If justice considerations moderate the relationship between attitude and behavior as it is theorized, then managers must also pay close attention to the processes in which policy is deliberated. Fairness judgments are an estimation of whether the right mix Of “fairness ingredients” have been incorporated into the process (Syme et al., 1999). Key to the success of a project is the effective communication that the process in which decisions were reached was both Open and transparent. Understanding the nature of attitudes is complicated by the lack Of a relationship between knowledge and behavior. The fact that knowledge is mediated by other psychological constructs complicates matters for two reasons. First, worldviews and attitudes were shown not to be dependent upon awareness of the issues or Of the action strategies, thus confounding the issue of what to include in a persuasive communication to people in the watershed. Second, if the direct path between knowledge and attitudes is muddy, then either contextual factors or other value domains (i.e. political ideology) or both must be having an influence. It is the managers job to uncover the degree to which each Of these might be involved in individual decision-making and make process adjustments to account for them so implementation goals can be set. 133 Recommendations for Future Research This study produced two important findings: (1) that attitudes appeared to be moderated by fairness judgments and (2) that the theory Of planned behavior was an effective tOOl for predicting behavioral intent. There were, however, some ways the results could be improved upon. First, the attitudinal factors need to be better defined. This researcher is confident the two factors that were derived indeed capture the essence of unique attitudes, but acknowledges there was collinearity between the other psychological variables that with some effort may be minimized. Focus groups and/or interviews may help in this regard while at the same time helping to better understand the local context. Second, knowledge was measured through the indexing Of several yes or no questions. Although this is an acceptable method Of eliciting information, it is possible that a more complex instrument might yield different results. Furthermore, this study tried to break down knowledge into two components: knowledge Of issues and knowledge of action strategies. This conceptual distinction in theory only yielded a minor difference in terms of responses. For these reasons, there needs to be further research into the structure of knowledge and its relationship with other psychological variables in environmental decision-making. Third, past studies have indicated that the American public may formulate their attitudes based upon political ideology (Steel et al., 1990; Steel, 1996). Given the apparent lack Of a relationship between knowledge and behavior and the weak relationship between knowledge and the other mediating variables Of attitudes, locus of control, and responsibility, it seems prudent for future studies to incorporate political items aimed at measuring respondent ideology. 134 And lastly, although considerable effort went into capturing the situational factors that were known to influence decision-making, this study was still limited in the specifies that were introduced. Variables such as location, accessibility and payment vehicles are all important factors not considered in this study. Future studies might wish to incorporate these variables. In all, the results Of this study serve as a starting point for better understanding Of how people think about managing their watershed. The study has been especially useful in isolating several issues needing attention when managing a watershed. 135 APPENDICES 136 Appendix A: Survey And Response Rate Data 137 OFFICE OF RESEARCH AND GRADUATE STUDIES 246 unhistatim Building Ea Lansing Michigan 48824-1046 517/355-21w FAX: 51 71353-2976 MICHIGAN STATE 0 N l v E R s 1 T Y June 26, 2001 TO: Scott G. WITTER 308 Natural Resources Building MSU RE: IRB if 00-567 CATEGORY: 2-G EXPEDITED TITLE: PREDICTING SUPPORT FOR BEST MANAGEMENT PRACTICES IN THE RED CEDAR WATERSHED BASED UPON INDIVIDUAL VALUES ANNUAL APPROVAL DATE: OCtOPOI' 10. 2000 REVISION REQUESTED: June 8. 2001 REVISION APPROVAL DATE: June 25. 2001 The University Committee on Research Involving Human Subjects' (UCRIHS) review of this project is complete and I am pleased to advise that the rights and welfare of the human subjects appear to be adequately protected and methods to Obtain informed consent are appropriate. Therefore. the UCRIHS APPROVED THIS PROJECT‘S REVISION. This letter approves the addition of the mail survey. RENEWALS: UCRIHS approval is valid for one calendar year, beginning with the approval date shown above. Projects continuing beyond one year must be renewed with the green renewal form. A maximum of four such expedited renewal are possible. Investigators wishing to continue a project beyond that time need to submit it again for a complete review. REVISIONS: UCRIHS must review any changes in procedures involving human subjects, prior to initiation of the change. If this is done at the time Of renewal. please use the green renewal form. . To revise an approved protocol at any other time during the year, send your written request to the UCRIHS Chair, requesting revised approval and referencing the project's IRB# and title. Include in your request a description of the change and any revised instruments, consent forms or advertisements that are applicable. PROBLEMS/CHANGES: Should either of the following arise during the course of the work, notify UCRIHS promptly: 1) problems (unexpected side effects. complaints, etc.) involving human subjects or 2) changes in the research environment or new information indicating greater risk to the human subjects than existed when the protocol was previously reviewed and approved. If we can be of further assistance, please contact us at 517 355-2180 or via email: Web. numeral/1mm * E—Mail: ucrihsOmsuedu mama-am 0&me Minuet “Slim WW UCRIHS@pilot.msu.edu. Sincerely, M 47/ M - UC I Ashir Kumar M. D. T3:§yé§:“%¥§" F93 Interim Chair UCRIHS ~21: w r. «IRES. 01.11" 1 0 200] . SUleraErtEWALAPPu ATI AK. bd . ONE oump pm 10300 0N cc: Stephen Pennington ABOVE DATE TO CONTINUE 1900 Pepper Tree Lane Lansing, MI 48912 a & a , ,, (beg Lisa 138 STATE MICHIGAN u N l R s I T v VE July 7?, 2001 Dear Mers Stakeholder: You are invited to participate in a study of the Red Cedar Watershed. The Red Cedar Watershed Coordinating Committee is a partnership of State, county and local governmental agencies together with the Department Of Resource Development at Michigan State University. Our mission is to work together to better manage the biophysical and socio- economic resources of the Red Cedar Watershed in a sustainable. fair, and cost-effective manner. The primary goal of the prOposed project is the creation of a watershed management plan that will be adopted by all community stakeholders. townships and county governments within the watershed. In order to foster widespread adoptiOn and maintenance of the best management practices we ask for your participation by completing and returning the enclosed survey in the return addressed envelope provided. Any information that is obtained in connection with this study will remain confidential. Only the investigators will have access to the raw data and any reports will convey information in the aggregate. Your privacy will be protected to the maximum extent allowable by the law. Your decision whether or not to participate in this research component will not prejudice your future relations with the Red Cedar Watershed Coordination Committee or Michigan State University. I O O O O O O O : ‘\‘ If you have any questions now or during the process. please feel free to contact us at one of : ‘ the numbers listed below. If you have any questions regarding your rights as a research , " participant, please contact the University Committee on Research involving Human Subjects 0 fl (UCRIHS) chair David E. Wright at 355-2180. ‘ 0 Your signature indicates that you have read the information provided above and are willing to coulseEOF participate. AGRICULTURE AND NATURAL Thank you very much for your time and consideration] RESOURCES Mira Develop-rod Participant new Star um - ”ninja”... Dr. Scott G. wttter Dr. Mike Kaplowitz Stephen Pennington East wig. man 319 Natural Resources Bldg 3113 Natural Resources Bldg 302 Natural Resources Bldg mum Michigan State University Michigan State University Michigan State University 517ml Phone (517) 355—3421 Phone (517) 355-0101 Phone (517) 355-3415 FAX: 51713530994 Fax (517) 353-8994 Fax (517) 353-8994 Fax (517) 353-8994 www.mmodu witter@msu.edu kalowitz@msu.edu pennin32@msu.edu UCRIHS APPROVAL FOR THIS prolect EXPIRES: OCT 1 0 2001 Wis-1W SUBMIT RENEWAL cadmium 139 ONEMClrl' Rafa???) ABOVE DATE TO counuue MICHIGAN STATE UNIVERSITY The Red Cedar River and its Streams i - N I POW 3"" 1 Shran‘ r ., C I _ _x Q7 _ _ _I’ """"""""" r """"""""" ------------ lilk'l'" I (‘n L X” ( a,“ Haslett i East Lansing ' Lanslng E . I 1 1,1..1In : ‘- ‘E I M] " her-:2“... 4’ Okcmos i g g '1 """"‘"’ ‘11,. Williamslon 2 i 1 r ”u. 5 4’ J, 1, "It \ 1 ° C,» ‘c We- . : Illa .fi ‘ i Fowlervllle 9 Red Cedar Rim ”'3'“ 3 g, ‘ _ 1 F 2‘ Finn: Lonqllt 40 miles ' 3 {E “In: How ‘72 to mlu "~ 3 I; " ’3” I! Tr, . Ii 1 a the Grand River in l ' 0 1 Stockbridge I t and air 1 I This is an Opportunity for you to provide information for local water resource planning. Your input will help planners make informed water quality decisions. Thank you for your participation. This booklet contains several sections Of brief questions that should take about 20 minutes to complete. Please return your completed questionnaire in the enclosed envelope to: Red Cedar Project - K, Department of Resource Development, 323 Natural Resources Building, Michigan State University, East Lansing, MI 48824-1222 140 Section 1: Uses And Thoughts About The Red Cedar River 1) How often do you do the following activities in the Red Cedar Area? (Mark E one response for each item) ' 2 ti 3 More Once a . o than 4 Never times a year times a year , , . . Z -.. _1 year a GO fishing [:1 Cl E [I b Use river water for lawns or gardens [:1 Cl E] [:1 c GO swimming Cl C] Cl C] d Irrigate crops [:1 E] C1 [I 6 Drain excess water into the river Cl E] El [:1 f Use river as dnnkrng supply for [:1 Cl E] E animals/pets Z Use area for nature walks or wrldl1fe E] E] El E1 vrewrng h :3; )recreattonal boating (canoe, kayak, E El [1 El i Use well water for household use E E Cl C] Use area for hunting [:1 El 1] E] k Other ( ) Cl [:1 El E1 2) In your opinion, how would you characterize the water quality of the Red Cedar River and it streams? (Please mark E one) [:1 Poor CI Fair B Good Cl Excellent [I Don’t know 3) Please read each statement below and indicate your level of agreement with each statement. (Please mark E one for each statement) activities that violate our water protection laws. e 22 :1 $.13 8’ a 8' E i: 0:: 8 ‘a E P = i a? a 8 .< 8 53- '4 It is my personal responsibility to protect our rivers a and streams for other peOple even if they seem E] El Cl E] El unconcerned. b It is not my responsibility to ensure the well being of El other species on earth. C I am partly responsible for the degraded state Of our local rivers and streams. d It is my responsibility to inform authorities about 141 streams support.... (Mark E one response for each activity) 4) In your opinion, how important is it to you that the Red Cedar River and its Not Somewhat Important Important Extremely Important Know Don’t Fishing [:1 E 1:] Cl Watering lawn/garden Swimming A drain for excess water Water supply for livestock/pets Nature appreciation Recreational Boating Hunting DEEEDEEE Other ( ) EEEDEEEE DEEEEEEE EEEEEEEE 5) Complete the following sentence with each of the statements below and indicate your level of agreement with each. (Mark E one response for each complete statement) 114)» individual actions would improve water g '3’ E 1 g > > g.’ qualityin local rivers and streams if I were to n: S of. E. "3 "3 s. g ".9. g 3 8 8 ".9. '< ""' '< attend a community meeting that involves El [:1 E! El El concern over our local streams and rivers. buy resource conservation devices, such as low- [:1 CI E E El flow faucet for my sinks and shower heads. report someone who violates a law or laws that protect our rivers and streams (e. g. illegal fishing, [3 [:1 Cl C] E polluting) to the prOper authorities. convince someone to sign a petition regarding E] El El E] El an issue surrounding our rivers and streams. ...convince someone to buy household cleaning and/or laundry products that don't harm the El E E CI [:1 environment. convince someone to conserve water by not running the water while brushing their teeth or El El E E E shaving and/or installing a water saving devices. 142 Section 2: Water Quality Concerns Experts have identified four concerns for the Red Cedar River and its streams. Pollution control prograrrrs are addressing some of these concerns. Other concerns require additional management practices. 2.1 Human and Animal Waste: Testing has found high levels of human and animal waste matter in the Red Cedar River and its streams. These levels make it unsafe to swim in the river 40% of the time. The problem of human and animal waste pollution is being addressed by 1) stormwater plans that separate sewer pipes from stormwater pipes, and 2) farm Operations adopting generally accepted agricultural management practices. 6) Have you heard about your community’s combined stormwater overflow plan? NO [I Yes [:1 7) Have you heard about farmers in your community adopting generally accepted management practices? No D Yes 1:] 2.2 N on-Point Source Pollution (N PS): Non-Point Source pollution) the result of such things as oil, gas, salts, fertilizers, pesticides, and other materials from homeowners, industry and agriculture being washed into surface waterbodies by rain or snowmelt. These pollutants are spread over wide areas and cannot be traced to a single source. While not visible, NPS pollution degrades water quality, impairs fish habitat and raises health concerns. 8) Have you heard about non-point source pollutants in the Red Cedar? NO [:1 \ Yes [:1 J 143 Water Quality Concerns (continued) (Blncreased Flow and Flooding: There has been increasing urbaD rural growth around the Red Cedar River and its streams. The additional hard surfaces (e. g. pavement) associated with urban and rural growth increases the amount and speedf Of water entering directly into the river and streams. Water moving directly into waterways destroys plant and animal habitat and causes flooding. 9) Are you aware of flooding problems in the Red Cedar or its streams? NO [:1 Yes D \ J arosion and Sedimentation: Erosion is the process of water washing soil into waterways. Sedimentation is the settling of soil particles on the bottom Of a water body (e.g. rivers, lakes). The removal of vegetative cover (e.g. forest, grass, brush, etc.) increases soil erosion and sedimentation. The murky water seen during and afier storm events is a result of erosion. Too much sedimentation can result in the loss of plant and animal habitat. One cause of erosion and sedimentation is construction in developing areas. 10) Are you aware of erosion prone areas of the Red Cedar River or its streams? No D Yes 1:] 144 Section 3: Best Management Practices. Experts recommend combinations of practices to improve water quality in the Red Cedar River system. The best management practices (BMPS) below control non-point source pollution, flooding and erosion. Other programs address animal and human waste concerns. All BMP projects include education programs for landowners, builders and Others in the community. Some BMPS use upland areas; others use lowland areas near the river and streams; while still others use land along stream banks. UPLAND BMPS Dry Basins: Dry basins are designed to hold runoff water. A dry basin allows water to seep into the ground and slows runoff into drainage systems. They empty after a storm event and are dry most of the time. Dry basins have a limited ability to remove soil particles and pollutants. Plans with MANY dry basins would have dry basins throughout available areas. Plans with SOME dry basins would have dry basins in only the most critical areas. 11) Have you seen a Dry Basin in your community? E No El Yes El Don’t know Wet Ponds: Wet ponds have a permanent pool of water and keep most stormwater runoff on site until it evaporates or seeps into the surrounding soils. Soil particles and some pollutants may settle in wet ponds Plans using MANY wet ponds would have wet ponds throughout available areas. Plans using SOME wet ponds would have wet ponds in only the most critical areas. 12) Have you seen a Wet Pond in your community? CI No I] Yes [:1 Don’t know J 145 LOWLAND BMPS r 1 Wetlands: Wetlands refer to areas with wet soil and the plants and animals that live there. Examples Of wetlands include wooded wetlands and marshes. Natural and man-made wetlands may filter pollutants, slow water flow and reduce flooding. Wetlands may be found near to rivers and streams. Plans with MANY wetlands would have wetlands in almost all available areas. Plans with SOME wetlands would have wetlands in only the most critical areas. 13) Have you seen a Wetland in your community? E] No El Yes Cl Don’t know L J Filter Strips: Filter strips are areas of land 5’ to 30’ wide near rivers and streams. Grasses, shrubs or trees can be used for filter strips. They allow stormwater to seep into the soil, reduce erosion and slow water entering rivers. Filter strips trap some pollutants. Plans with MANY filter ships would have strips in almost all available areas. Plans with SOME filter strips would have strips in only the most critical areas. 14) Have you seen a Filter Strip in your community? [:1 No [:1 Yes CI Don’t know L J 146 STREAM BANK BMPS r 1 Rip Rap: Rip Rap are large stones placed along stream banks and stormwater inlets to protect them from flowing water. Rip Rap slows down water flow and reduces stream bank erosion and sedimentation. Rip Rap does not usually remove pollutants other than erosion. Plans with MANY rip raps would have rip rap along almost all available stream banks. Plans with SOME rip raps would have rip rap along only the most critical stream banks. CS) Have you seen Rip Rap in your community? El No El Yes [:1 Don’t knovD W Streambank Naturalization: Streambank natural-ization uses native grasses, plants, trees, rocks, and tree stumps to rebuild the banks Of streams or rivers. Streambank natural-ization Slows runoff, traps pollutants and sediments and allows water to seep into the soil. Plans with MANY naturalizations would have it along almost all available stream banks. Plans with SOME naturalizations would have it along only the most critical stream banks. 16) Have you seen Streambank Naturalization in your community? El No D Yes [I Don’t Know 147 Section 4: BMP Combinations Additional BMPS Needed fl adequately protect water quality. Additional BMPS such as dry basins, wet (The current management practices in the Red Cedar River area do no ponds, filter strips, wetlands, rip rap and stream bank naturalization will improve water quality. There are areas available for additional BMPS in the Red Cedar River area. in J Combining BMPS r n Individual BMPS are combined together to improve water quality. Water resource experts recommend different combinations of BMPS. For the Red Cedar River and its streams, several different combinations can improve the Red Cedar’s water quality so that it will become swirnable and fishable. L J Achieve Same Water Quality f R BMP combinations are known as plans. Each plan suggested by a panel of experts will help achieve the same water quality for the Red Cedar. However, each Of the plans differ in the particular BMPS they use. That is, each plan uses different levels of the six possible BMPS discussed above in éwtron 3. J Same Cost to Community r 1 Each of the proposed plans is estimated to cost the same amount to your community. That is, each plan will result in the same water quality improvements, cost the same to your community and use different BMPs. L J 148 Section 5: Implementation Information The use Of BMPS are Often part of larger programs to improve water quality and protect natural resources. We would like your input on some complimentary elements and approaches that may be used for improving water quality in the Red Cedar River and its streams. 20) In your opinion, how supportive would you be of the following? (Mark E one response for each item) Would Might Would Strongly Do Not Support Support Support Not Support g, . gKgnow Zoning requirements for some a Open space to be preserved on Cl C] E C] E undeveloped land. Subsidies to landowners for b environmentally friendly [:1 E El E El practices. Stricter regulations on activities c that impact waterways during [:1 Cl C] E] El develOpment. d Fines for polluting. Cl C] E] Cl C] Increased enforcement of E] El 1:] (:1 El envrronmental regulations. Public information and education [:1 [:1 El E] El programs. Voluntary programs to help landowners adopt environmentally fiiendly D D D D [:1 practices. 21) Mark your three preferred information sources about water quality issues in the Red Cedar River and its streams? Michigan Department of Agriculture Natural Resource Conservation Service Michigan State University (other than Extension) Michigan State University Extension Michigan Department Of Environmental Quality Soil Conservation District Drain Commissioners Farm Bureau County Health Department Michigan Environmental Council Local Newspapers Broadcast Media TV/Radio El Other ( ) CID EEEEEEEEDE 149 Section 6: Attitudes and Beliefs Designing and implementing water quality plans can be improved when managers understand the Opinions of the people living in the area. To help give policy makers and planners for the Red Cedar River a better understanding of how you feel, please answer the following questions. 22) For the statements below, please share with us your opinion on how much you agree with each. (Mark E one response for each item) a a, 9. 2 a at s s a is it: a a E a = s s a 8 3< . 8 9;. 5< a All members of the communrty have a rrght to then say E E E C] E on 1ssues involvrng water management. Everyone may have to make some personal sacrifices if b we are going to have effective water resource El El [:1 Cl Cl programs. You cannot really solve water pollution problems by c analyzing the costs and benefits in dollars. E] D D D D EveryOne owns rivers and streams and therefore they d should be managed for the overall public benefit. D D D D D 3 Those who pollute the most should pay the greatest El E] Cl C] I] share of clean up and protection costs. f {he envrronment has the same rrght to water as people 1:] E] D E] El ave. g Public involvement should. not be part Of the decrsron- Cl Cl Cl [:1 El making process for managing water resources. h Water pollution programs should be made tomaxrmrze Cl C] E E El the overall economrc income of the communrty. The long-term environmental health Of local waterways i should be achieved even if it reduces short-term E] E 1:] El E1 business profits. j Water has value other than its dollar value. [:1 Cl E E E 150 aerfiesrq Alfiuorrg aorfiesrq 113-linen oarfiv as: v Alfiuorrs The government does not need to be involved in cleaning up and protecting water resources. E Cl C] E E If we are to clean up and protect our rivers, it should be done so as to minimize conflict in communities. [:1 El [:1 E El Those upstream have a moral responsibility to look m after the interests of those downstream. '3 D D D D u If the decrsron—makrng process is farr, people should E] Cl C] E] El accept 1ts decrsrons for addressrng water pollution. If new water pollution programs hurt some peOple's o livelihoods, they should receive compensation. D D D D D p Groundwater (water under land) is the property of the [:1 Cl Cl Cl C] landowner above. q When rt comes to clean waterways, the envrronment E [:1 E Cl C] should be a secondary consideration to peOple. Saving waterways for the firture is more important than [:1 Cl [:1 E] El making money now. While some parts Of the natural environment are s valuable and should be preserved, some are not so [:1 E] Cl [:1 El valuable and should not be preserved. t There rs not time to wart for exact envrronmental E] CI [:1 Cl C] knowledge, we need to act now. Section 7: Demographics 23) How long have you lived in the Ingham/Livingston County area? years 24) How long have you lived in your current home? years 151 25) Do you currently own or rent your home? [:1 Own [:1 Rent 26) Which category includes your age? 18 to 24 years 25 to 34 years 35 to 44 years 45 to 54 years 55 to 64 years 65 + DECIDED 27) Please indicate your highest level of education. CI Less than High School Graduate El High School or GED [:1 Some College El College Graduate El Post Graduate Degree 28) Which category includes your household’s gross income? Less than $10,000 $10,000 - $14,999 $15,000 — $19,999 $20,000 — $29,999 $30,000 — $39,999 $40,000 - $49,999 $50,000 — $59,999 $60,000 — $69,999 $70,000 + EEEEEEEE Cl 29) How many adults (18 years of age and over) currently live in your household? 30) How many children under 18 currently live in your household? 152 F eel free to use the back cover if you have any further comments. Thank you for your time! Please place the survey in the envelope provided and return it to: Red Cedar Project, Department of Resource Development, 323 Natural Resources Building, Michigan State University, East Lansing, MI 48824-1222 Comments: Original number of surveys 1650 wave 1 wave 2 wave 3 (Choice Exp. Only) undeliverable 21 1 5 10 Return to Sender (RTS) 15 8 10 Blanks 9 10 10 returns 423 185 Total 608 response rate (rr) 26.0% 11.3% wave rr 26% 16% response rate 0.3767 rr if RTS = undeliverable 0.3943 153 Appendix B: Response Frequencies For Survey Questions 154 Standard Deviation and Variance for all Valid N ' ' Mean Std. Deviation 1.5 1.6 1.3 1.57 1. 1.64 1.4 1.7 1.5 1.831 1.4 1. 2. 1.5 l. 1.65 1.5 1.74 1.4 1.69 3. 4. 4.41 2. 4.1 2. 2.5 2.5 3 3. 3. 2.5 2.6 3.3 3.6 4. 3.5 3. 3.7 -.51 .6 -.l 3.5 2.3 1.4 3. 1. 3.1 2. —l y—ag—sp—er—du—s—sg—s O 155 Mean Std. Deviation 2.] 2.201 1. l. 1.57 1.25 1.43 1.4 1.45 4.1 . .85 4. . .7 3.6 1.65 4. . 1.04 4 4 .8 .73 . 5 1.34 4. .11 1.2 3.2 . 2.09 3.9 . 1.25 4. . . 4.1 . 1.2 3. . 1.33 4.21 . 1.35 4. . 1. 3.41 . 1.91 2. . 2.45 2. . 2.861 4.1 . 1.3 3.5 . 1.88 3.7 . 1.91 37.995 539.352] 20.829 22.0846] 487.7300 156 Valid N ' ' Mean Std. Deviation Variance l. 1. 2.13 4. 1.58 2.51 3.61 1.43 2 7.7 4.561 20. 2. 1.58 2.5 .7 1.83 3.38 Frequencies Qla Flow often do you go fishin r in the Red Cedar River? Frguency Percent Valid Percent Cumulative % Never 513 84.4 84.4 84.4 Once a Year 26 4.3 4.3 88.7 2 to 3 times a yr 29 4.8 4.8 93.4 > 4 times a year 17 2.8 2.8 96.2 no response 23 3.8 3.8 100.0 Total 608 100.0 100.0 lb THow often do you use water from the Red Cedar River for your lawn or garden? Frequency Percent Valid Percent Cumulative % Never 566 93.1 93.1 93.] Once a Year 1 .2 .2 93.3 2 to 3 times a yr 9 1.5 1.5 94.7 > 4 times a year 9 1.5 1.5 96.2 no response 23 3.8 3.8 100.0 Total 608 100.0 100.0 1c THow Often do you go swimming in Red Cedar River? Frequency Percent Valid Percent Cumulative % Never 547 90.0 90.0 90.0 Once a Year 14 2.3 2.3 92.3 2tO3timesayr 11 1.8 1.8 94.1 > 4 times a year 11 1.8 1.8 95.9 no response 25 4.1 4.1 100.0 Total 608 100.0 100.0 1d [How often do you irrigate your crops with water from the Red Cedar River? Frequency Percent Valid Percent Cumulative % Never 573 94.2 94.2 94.2 2 to 3 times a yr 1 .2 .2 94.4 > 4 times a year 4 .7 .7 95.1 no response 30 4.9 4.9 100.0 Total 608 100.0 100.0 157 Qle Jl-Iow Often do you drain excess water into the Red Cedar River? Frequency Percent Valid Percent Cumulative % Never 559 91.9 91.9 91.9 2 to 3 times a yr 3 .5 .5 92.4 > 4 times a year 14 2.3 2.3 94.7 no response 32 5.3 5.3 100.0 Total 608 100.0 100.0 Qlf [How Often do you use the Red Cedar River as drinking supply for animals/pets? Frequency Percent Valid Percent Cumulative % Never 560 92.1 92.1 92.] Once a Yr 2 .3 .3 92.4 2 t0 3 “ms 3 7 1.2 1.2 93.6 year > 4 times a year 8 1.3 1.3 94.9 no response 31 5.] 5.1 100.0 Total 608 100.0 100.0 1g Il-low Often do use the Red Cedar River area for nature walks or wildlife viewing? Frequency Percent Valid Percent Cumulative % Never 165 27.1 27.1 27.] Once a Year 90 14.8 14.8 41.9 2 to 3 times a yr 139 22.9 22.9 64.8 > 4 times a year 197 32.4 32.4 97.2 no response 17 2.8 2.8 100.0 Total 608 100.0 100.0 lh [How Often do you go recreational boating in the Red Cedar River? Frequency Percent Valid Percent Cumulative % Never 442 72.7 72.7 72.7 Once a Year 74 12.2 12.2 84.9 2tO3timesayr 43 7.1 7.1 91.9 > 4 times a year 26 4.3 4.3 96.2 no response 23 3.8 3.8 100.0 Total 608 100.0 100.0 Qli jI-Iow often do you use well water for household use? Frequency Percent Valid Percent Cumulative % Never 539 88.7 88.7 88.7 2 to 3 times a yr 2 .3 .3 89.0 >4 timesayear 4] 6.7 6.7 95.7 no response 26 4.3 4.3 100.0 Total 608 100.0 100.0 158 Q] j [How Often do you use the Red Cedar River area for hunting? Frequency Percent Valid Percent Cumulative % Never 540 88.8 88.8 88.8 Once a Year 10 1.6 1.6 90.5 2 tO 3 times a yr 15 2.5 2.5 92.9 > 4 times a year 17 2.8 2.8 95.7 no response 26 4.3 4.3 100.0 Total 608 100.0 100.0 Qlk [How often do you the Red Cedar River or surrounding area for other activities? Frequency Percent Valid Cumulative % Percent Blank 585 96.2 96.2 96.2 Bicycling 1 .2 .2 96.4 'Biking Qx) l .2 .2 96.5 Bird Watching (4x) 1 .2 .2 96.7 Bird, wildlife watching 1 .2 .2 96.9 Cross-country skiing (4x) 1 .2 .2 97.0 Dogs go swimming (>4x) ] .2 .2 97.2 Drive over (4x) 1 .2 .2 97.4 xercise (4x) 1 .2 .2 97.5 Flood relief (4x) 1 .2 .2 97.7 Golfing (4x) 1 .2 .2 97.9 'Mountain biking (4x) 1 .2 .2 98.0 Other (3x) 1 .2 .2 98.2 Other (4x) 1 .2 .2 98.4 Photo opts (4x) 1 .2 .2 98.5 River trail for biking (4x) 1 .2 .2 98.7 Running, walking (4x) 1 .2 .2 98.8 Scenic/Bird watching (4x) 1 .2 .2 99.0 Sight seeing, hiking (1x) 1 .2 .2 99.2 Visit city overlooks (3x) 1 .2 .2 99.3 Wading (4x) 1 .2 .2 99.5 Walk doggsit by MSU (4x) 1 .2 .2 99.7 Walks at MSU (3x) 1 .2 .2 99.8 XX skiing, snowshoeing (4x) 1 .2 .2 100.0 Total 608 100.0 100.0 159 eams Q2 [How would you characterize the water quality of the Red Cedar River and its st Frequency Percent Valid Percent Cumulative % Poor 145 23.8 23.8 23.8 Fair 198 32.6 32.6 56.4 Good 90 14.8 14.8 71.2 Excellent 5 .8 .8 72.0 Don't Know 150 24.7 24.7 96.7 NO Response 20 3.3 3.3 100.0 Total 608 100.0 100.0 Q33 It is my personal responsibility to protect our rivers and streams for other pe0p1e even if they seem unconcerned. Frequency Percent Valid Percent Cumulative %t Sirong‘y 8 1.3 1.3 1.3 Drsagree Disagree 26 4.3 4.3 5.6 Neutral 104 17.1 17.1 22.7 Agree 316 52.0 52.0 74.7 Strongly Agree 136 22.4 22.4 97.0 NO Response 18 3.0 3.0 100.0 Total 608 100.0 100.0 Q3b- It is not my responsibility to ensure the well being of other species on earth. recoded Freguency Percent Valid Percent Cumulative % Stimg'y 4 .7 .7 .7 Drsagree Disagree 29 4.8 4.8 5.4 Neutral 52 8.6 8.6 14.0 Agree 242 39.8 39.8 53.8 Strongly Agree 259 42.6 42.6 96.4 No Response 22 3.6 3.6 100.0 Total 608 100.0 100.0 Q3c I I am partly responsible for the degraded state of our local rivers and stream. Frequency Percent Valid Percent Cumulative % Simg'y 97 16.0 16.0 16.0 Drsagree Disagree 144 23.7 23.7 39.6 Neutral 169 27.8 27.8 67.4 Agree 156 25.7 25.7 93.1 Strongly Agree 19 3.1 3.1 96.2 NO Response 23 3.8 3.8 100.0 Total 608 100.0 100.0 I60 It is my responsibility to inform authorities about activities that violate our water Q3d protection laws. Frequency Percent Valid Percent Cumulative % Simngly 9 1.5 1.5 1.5 Drsagree Disagree 16 2.6 2.6 4.] Neutral 67 11.0 11.0 15.1 Agree 333 54.8 54.8 69.9 Strongly Agree 168 27.6 27 .6 97.5 No Response 15 2.5 2.5 100.0 Total 608 100.0 100.0 How important to you is it that the Red Cedar River and its stream support Q43 fi h' 2 s mg. F reguency Percent Valid Percent Cumulative % Not lrnportant 72 11.8 11.8 11.8 Somewhat Important 287 47.2 47 .2 59.0 Extremely Important 207 34.0 34.0 93.] Don't Know 27 4.4 4.4 97.5 No Response 15 2.5 2.5 100.0 Total 608 100.0 100.0 Q 4b How important to you is it that the Red Cedar River and its stream support wateringlawns and gardens? Frequency Percent Valid Percent Cumulative % Not Important 252 41.4 41.4 41.4 Somewhat lmpo rtan t 222 36.5 36.5 78.0 Extremely Important 49 8.1 8.] . 86.0 Don't Know 67 11.0 11.0 97.0 No Response 18 3.0 3.0 100.0 Total 608 100.0 100.0 Q40 How important to you is it that the Red Cedar River and its stream support swimming? Frequency Percent Valid Percent Cumulative % Not Important 182 29.9 29.9 29.9 Somewhat Important 252 41.4 41.4 71.4 Extremely Important 1 13 18.6 18.6 90.0 Don't Know 44 7.2 7.2 97.2 No Response 17 2.8 2.8 100.0 Total 608 100.0 100.0 161 Q4 d How important to you is it that the Red Cedar River and its stream support draining excess water? Frequency Percent Valid Percent Cumulative % Not Important 88 14.5 14.5 14.5 Somewhat ”@0113!“ 208 34.2 34.2 48.7 Extremely Important 208 34.2 34.2 82.9 Don't Know 88 14.5 14.5 97.4 NO Response 16 2.6 2.6 100.0 Total 608 100.0 100.0 - How important to you is it that the Red Cedar River and its stream be a water Q4e . supply for livestock or pets? Frequency Percent Valid Percent Cumulative % Not Important 124 20.4 20.4 20.4 Somewhat Important 269 44.2 44.2 64.6 Extremely Important 109 1 7.9 17.9 82.6 Don't Know 90 14.8 14.8 97.4 NO Response 16 2.6 2.6 100.0 Total 608 100.0 100.0 Q4 f How important to you is it that the Red Cedar River and its stream support nature appreciation? Frequency Percent Valid Percent Cumulative % Not Important 1] 1.8 1.8 1.8 Somewhat Important 138 22.7 22.7 24.5 Extremely Important 429 70.6 70.6 95. 1 Don't Know 21 3.5 3.5 98.5 NO Response 9 1.5 1.5 100.0 Total 608 100.0 100.0 How important to you is it that the Red Cedar River and its stream support Q4g recreational boating? Frequency Percent Valid Percent Cumulative % Not Important 88 14.5 14.5 14.5 Somewhat Important 294 48.4 48.4 62.8 Extremely Important 184 30.3 30.3 93. 1 Don't Know 29 4.8 4.8 97.9 NO Response 13 2.] 2.1 100.0 Total 608 100.0 100.0 162 How important to you is it that the Red Cedar River and its stream support Q4h hunting? Frequency Percent Valid Percent Cumulative % Not Important 219 36.0 36.0 36.0 Somewhat Important 225 37.0 37.0 73.0 Extremely Important 79 13 .0 13.0 86.0 Don't Know 69 11.3 11.3 97.4 NO Response 16 2.6 2.6 100.0 Total 608 100.0 100.0 How important to you is it that the Red Cedar River and its stream support other Q‘“ activities? Frequency Percent Valid Percent Cumulative % 583 95.9 95.9 95.9 Clean environment for Wildlife (3) 1 .2 .2 96.] Disease control (3) l .2 .2 96.2 [Habitat for wildlife (2) 1 .2 .2 98.0 [Hiking L3) 1 .2 .2 98.2 Illiking, rec. use (2) I .2 .2 98.4 “nake attractive ptS of l .2 .2 98.5 ountain biking (3) l .2 .2 98.7 ature Trial (3) 1 .2 .2 98.8 Support wildlife (3) 1 .2 .2 99.7 Sustainable env. (3) 1 .2 .2 99.8 The view (3) 1 .2 .2 100.0 Total 608 100.0 100.0 2 = Somewhat Important, 3 = Extremely Important My individual actions would improve water quality in local rivers and streams if Q 5 a I were to attend a community meeting that involves concern over our local streams and rivers. Frequency Percent Valid Percent Cumulative % Slmngly 23 3.8 3.8 3.8 Drsagree Disagree 90 14.8 14.8 18.6 Neutral 238 39.1 39.1 57.7 Agree 223 36.7 36.7 94.4 Strongly Agree 22 3.6 3.6 98.0 NO Response 12 2.0 2.0 100.0 Total 608 100.0 100.0 163 My individual actions would improve water quality in local rivers and streams if QSb I were to buy resource conservation devices such as. Frequency Percent Valid Percent Cumulative % Sirong'y 19 3.1 3.1 3.1 Drsagree Disagree 74 12.2 12.2 15.3 Neutral 145 23.8 23.8 39.] Agree 297 48.8 48.8 88.0 Strongly Agree 60 9.9 9.9 97 .9 No Response 13 2.1 2.1 100.0 Total 608 100.0 100.0 My individual actions would improve water quality in local rivers and streams if Q5c I were report someone who violates a law or laws that protect our rivers and streams to the prOper authorities. Frequency Percent Valid Percent Cumulative % Sirongly 5 .8 .8 .8 Drsagree Disagree 8 1.3 1.3 2.] Neutral 53 8.7 8.7 10.9 Agree 342 56.3 56.3 67.] Strongly Agree 190 31.3 31.3 98.4 NO Response 10 1.6 1.6 100.0 Total 608 100.0 100.0 My individual actions would improve water quality in local rivers and streams if Q5d l were convince someone to Sign a petition regarding an issue surrounding our rivers and streams. Frequency Percent Valid Percent Cumulative % 31mg” 14 2.3 2.3 2.3 Drsagree Disagree 48 7 .9 7.9 10.2 Neutral 266 43.8 43.8 53.9 Agree 225 37.0 37.0 91.0 Strongly Agree 43 7.1 7.1 98.0 NO Response 12 2.0 2.0 100.0 Total 608 100.0 100.0 164 My individual actions would improve water quality in local rivers and streams if Q5e I were convince someone tO buy household cleaning and/or laundry products that don’t harm the environment. Frequency Percent Valid Percent Cumulative % 80°“le 6 1.0 1.0 1.0 Drsagree Disagree 27 4.4 4.4 5.4 Neutral 116 19.] 19.] 24.5 Agree 352 57.9 57.9 82.4 Strongly Agree 96 15.8 15.8 98.2 NO Response 11 1.8 1.8 100.0 Total 608 100.0 100.0 My individual actions would improve water quality in local rivers and streams if Q5f I were convince someone to conserve water by not running the water while brushing their teeth or shaving and/or installing a water saving device. Frequency Percent Valid Percent Cllt)mulatlve ercent Sirong‘y 17 2.8 2.8 2.8 Drsagree Disagree 47 7.7 7.7 10.5 Neutral 135 22.2 22.2 32.7 Agree 306 50.3 50.3 83.1 Strongly Agree 92 15.1 15.] 98.2 NO Response 1] 1.8 1.8 100.0 Total 608 100.0 100.0 Q6 1 Have ou heard about your community’s combined stormwater overflow plan? Frequency Percent Valid Percent Cumulative % no 226 37.2 37.2 37.2 yes 374 61.5 61.5 98.7 NO Response 8 1.3 1.3 100.0 Total 608 100.0 100.0 Q7 Have you heard about farmers in your community adopting generally accepted management practices? Frequency Percent Valid Percent Cumulative % no 516 84.9 84.9 84.9 yes 78 12.8 12.8 97.7 NO Response 14 2.3 2.3 100.0 Total 608 100.0 100.0 165 Q8 IHave you heard about non- Oint sourceiollutants in the Red Cedar? Frequency Percent Valid Percent Cumulative % no 350 57.6 57.6 57.6 yes 248 40.8 40.8 98.4 NO Response 10 1.6 1.6 100.0 Total 608 1 00.0 100.0 Q9 [Are you aware of flooding oroblems in the Red Cedar or its streams? Frequency Percent Valid Percent Cumulative % no 14] 23.2 23.2 23.2 yes 461 75.8 75.8 99.0 NO Response 6 1.0 1.0 100.0 Total 608 100.0 100.0 Q10 I Are you aware of erosion prone areas of the Red Cedar River or its streams? Frequency Percent Valid Percent Cumulative % no 379 62.3 62.3 62.3 yes 221 36.3 36.3 98.7 NO Response 8 1.3 1.3 100.0 Total 608 100.0 100.0 Q11 I Have you seen a DIyBasin in your community? Frequency Percent Valid Percent Cumulative % no 169 27.8 27.8 27.8 yes 175 28.8 28.8 56.6 Don't Know 257 42.3 42.3 98.8 NO Response 7 1.2 1.2 100.0 Total 608 100.0 100.0 Q12 I Have you seen a Wet Pond in your communi ? Frequency Percent Valid Percent Cumulative 5 no 121 19.9 19.9 19.9 yes 338 55.6 55.6 75.5 Don't Know 142 23.4 23.4 98.8 No Response 7 1.2 1.2 100.0 Total 608 100.0 100.0 Q13 I Have you seen a Wetland in your community? Frequency Percent Valid Percent Cumulative % no 58 9.5 9.5 9.5 yes 495 81.4 81.4 91.0 Don't Know 51 8.4 8.4 99.3 NO Response 4 .7 .7 100.0 Total 608 100.0 100.0 166 Q14 IHave you seen a Filter Strip in your community? Frequency Percent Valid Percent Cumulative % no 161 26.5 26.5 26.5 yes 193 31.7 31.7 58.2 Don't Know 25] 41.3 41.3 99.5 NO Response 3 .5 .5 100.0 Total 608 100.0 100.0 Q15 I Have you seen Rip Rap in your community? Frequency Percent Valid Percent Cumulative % no 207 34.0 34.0 34.0 yes 257 42.3 42.3 76.3 Don't Know 14] 23.2 23.2 99.5 NO Response 3 .5 .5 100.0 Total 608 100.0 100.0 Q16 I Have you seen Streambank Naturalization in our community? Frequency Percent Valid Percent Cumulative % no 125 20.6 20.6 20.6 gyes 262 43.1 43.] 63.7 Don't Know 217 35.7 35.7 99.3 NO Response 4 .7 .7 100.0 Total 608 100.0 100.0 C17 TWhich of these two plans would you prefer in your community? Frequency Percent Valid Percent Cumulative % Plan A 279 45.9 45 .9 45.9 Plan B 282 46.4 46.4 92.3 NO Response 47 7 .7 7.7 100.0 Total 608 100.0 100.0 C18 I Which of these two plans woulclyou prefer in your community? Frequency Percent Valid Percent Cumulative % Plan A 231 38.0 38.0 38.0 Plan B 321 52.8 52.8 90.8 NO Response 56 9.2 9.2 100.0 Total 608 100.0 100.0 C19 I Which of these two plans would you prefer in your community? Frequency Percent Valid Percent Cumulative % Plan A 268 44.1 44.1 44.] Plan B 285 46.9 46.9 91.0 No Response 55 9.0 9.0 100.0 Total 608 100.0 100.0 167 How supportive would you be of zoning requirements for some Open space to be Q2021 preserved on undeveloped land? Frequency Percent Valid Percent Cumulative % WW“ N“ 21 3.5 3.5 3.5 Support Might Support 98 16.1 16.1 19.6 Would Supmrt 193 31.7 31.7 51.3 Strongly Support 239 39.3 39.3 90.6 Do Not Know 36 5.9 5.9 96.5 NO Response 21 3.5 3.5 100.0 Total 608 100.0 100.0 How supportive would you be of subsidies to landowners for environmentally Q20b fii . endly practlces? Frequency Percent Valid Percent Cumulative % WW“ N‘” 53 8.7 8.7 8.7 Support Might Simport 163 26.8 26.8 35.5 Would Support 216 35.5 35.5 71.] Strongly Support 1 18 19.4 19.4 90.5 Do Not Know 39 6.4 6.4 96.9 NO Response 19 3.] 3.1 100.0 Total 608 100.0 100.0 How supportive would you be Of stricter regulations on activities that impact QZOc . waterways durrng development? Frequency Percent Valid Percent Cumulative % WW“ N“ 16 2.6 2.6 2.6 Support Might Support 65 10.7 10.7 13.3 Would Support 238 39.] 39.1 52.5 Strongly Support 24] 39.6 39.6 92.] DO Not Know 30 4.9 4.9 97.0 NO Response 18 3.0 3.0 100.0 Total 608 100.0 100.0 168 Q20g How sup aortive would you be of fines for polluting? Frequency Percent Valid Percent Cumulative % W°“'d N“ 10 1.6 1.6 1.6 Support Might Support 42 6.9 6.9 8.6 Would Support 151 24.8 24.8 33.4 Strongly Support 376 61.8 61.8 95.2 DO Not Know 16 2.6 2.6 97.9 NO Response 13 2.1 2.] 100.0 Total 608 100.0 100.0 Q20e How supportive would you be of increased enforcement of environmental regulations? Frequency Percent Valid Percent Cumulative % would N“ 21 3.5 3.5 3.5 Support Might Support 60 9.9 9.9 13.3 Would Support 206 33.9 33.9 47.2 Strongly Support 286 47.0 47.0 94.2 DO Not Know 19 3.1 3.1 97.4 NO Response 16 2.6 2.6 100.0 Total 608 100.0 100.0 Q20f [How sup aortive would you be public information and education programs? Frequency Percent Valid Percent Cumulative % WW“ N‘” 12 2.0 2.0 2.0 Support Might Support 75 12.3 12.3 14.3 Would Support 260 42.8 42.8 57 .1 Strongly Support 227 37.3 37.3 94.4 DO Not Know 17 2.8 2.8 97.2 NO Response 17 2.8 2.8 100.0 Total 608 100.0 100.0 Q20g How supportive would you be voluntary programs tO help landowners adopt environmentally fiiendly practices? Frequency Percent Valid Percent Cumulative % WW“ 1"“ 9 1.5 1.5 1.5 Support Might Support 72 11.8 11.8 13.3 Would Support 278 45.7 45.7 59.0 Strongly Support 210 34.5 34.5 93.6 DO Not Know 23 3.8 3.8 97.4 NO Response 16 2.6 2.6 100.0 Total 608 100.0 100.0 169 Q21 I Three preferred information sources: Organization Frequency Ranking MDA 168 4 NRCS 164 6 MSU 119 7 MSUE 166 5 MDEQ 206 3 Soil CD 75 10 Drain Commission 86 9 Farm Bureau 27 12 CO. Health Dept. 114 8 MEC 58 11 Newspapers 226 2 TV/Radio 232 1 Others, MUCC, Ml Pork Producers, Sierra Club, EPA, PIRGIM, Individual Mailings Questions 22 a-t; Please see summary Table 10 or Appendix E. Question 23 & 24 see mean, standard deviation and variances above. Q25 IDO you currently own or rent your home? Frequency Percent Valid Percent Cumulative % Own 587 96.5 96.5 96.5 NO Response 21 3.5 3.5 100.0 Total 608 100.0 100.0 Q26 I Which category includes your age? Frequency Percent Valid Percent Cumulative % 18-24 yrs 6 1.0 1.0 1.0 25 - 34 yrs 79 13.0 13.0 14.0 35 - 44 yrs 119 19.6 19.6 33.6 45 - 54 yrs 155 25.5 25.5 59.0 55 - 64 yrs 107 17.6 17.6 76.6 65 + yrs 123 20.2 20.2 96.9 NO Regponse 19 3.1 3.1 100.0 Total 608 100.0 100.0 170 Q27 I Please indicate your highest level of education. Frequency Percent Valid Percent Cumulative % < High Scool Graduate 20 3.3 3.3 3.3 Some School or GED 10] 16.6 16.6 19.9 Some College 179 29.4 29.4 49.3 College Graduate 181 29.8 29.8 79.] P0“ graduate 108 17.8 17.8 96.9 Degree NO Response 19 3.1 3.] 100.0 Total 608 100.0 100.0 Q28 I Which catego includes your household income? Frequency Percent Valid Percent Cumulative % <$10,000 /yr 13 2.] 2.1 2.1 10,000 - 14,999 / yr 13 2.1 2.] 4.3 15,000 - 19,999 /yr 18 3.0 3.0 7.2 20,000 - 29,999 /yr 64 10.5 10.5 17.8 30,000 - 39,999 /yr 92 15.1 15.1 32.9 40,000 - 49,999 /yr 88 14.5 14.5 47.4 50,000 - 59,999 /yr 77 12.7 12.7 60.0 60,000 - 69,999 /yr 59 9.7 9.7 69.7 70,000 + /yr 112 18.4 18.4 88.2 NO Response 72 11.8 1 1.8 100.0 Total 608 100.0 100.0 Q29 I How many adults (1 8 years of age and over) currently live in your household? Frequency Percent Valid Percent Cumulative % 0 27 4.4 4.4 4.4 l 172 28.3 28.3 32.7 2 339 55.8 55.8 88.5 3 36 5.9 5.9 94.4 4 7 1.2 1.2 95.6 5 2 .3 .3 95.9 6 2 .3 .3 96.2 NO Regionse 23 3.8 3.8 100.0 Total 608 100.0 100.0 I71 Q30 I How many children under 18 currently live in your household? Frequency Percent Valid Percent Cumulative % 0 431 70.9 70.9 70.9 1 72 11.8 11.8 82.7 2 58 9.5 9.5 92.3 3 20 3.3 3.3 95.6 4 3 .5 .5 96.1 5 1 .2 .2 96.2 No Response 23 3.8 3.8 100.0 Total 608 100.0 100.0 172 Appendix C: Exploratory Factor Analysis Output 173 174 Extraction Method: Principal Component Analysis with Varimax Rotation Descriptive Statistics Communalities Mean Std. Analysis N Initial Extraction Deviation Q22A l .000 .5 ] 7 Q22A 4.07 .655 538 Q22B 1.000 .465 QZZB 4.00 .666 538 Q22C 1.000 .488 Q22C 3.51 .965 538 Q22D 1.000 .431 Q22D 3.97 .812 538 Q22E 1.000 .385 Q22E 4.39 .675 538 Q22F 1.000 .54] Q22F 3.99 .859 538 Q22GJ RC 1.000 .637 Q22G_RC 3.98 .863 538 Q27-H 1-000 -625 Q22H 3.05 .948 538 Q22I 1.000 .528 Q22] 3.83 .773 538 Q22J 1.000 .412 Q22] 4.35 .728 538 Q22K 1.000 .404 Q22K 1.93 .890 538 Q22L 1.000 .548 Q22L 3.51 .853 538 Q22M 1.000 .401 @M 4.04 .777 538 Q22N 1.000 .531 Q22N 3.85 .666 538 Q220 1.000 .534 Q220 3.19 .838 538 (222P 1-000 -387 Q22P 2.66 .992 5 38 Q22Q l .000 .442 Q22Q 2.31 .906 538 Q22K 1.000 .516 Q22R 3.99 .793 5 38 Q228 1.000 .693 Q22S 2.59 .945 5 38 Q22T 1.000 .367 Q22T 3.52 .952 538 Total Variance Explained . . Extraction Rotation Sums Inltlal Sums of Bi enval s uar d °f 3‘19”“ g ues q .e Loadlngs Loadlngs %Of Cumulative %Of Cumulative %Of Cumulative Component Total Variance % Total Variance % Total Variance % 1 4.190 20.952 20.952 4.190 20.952 20.952 2.745 13.727 13.727 2 1.975 9.874 30.825 1.975 9.874 30.825 2.222 11.112 24.839 3 1.440 7.198 38.023 1.440 7.198 38.023 1.756 8.78] 33.621 4 1.217 6.083 44.106 1.217 6.083 44.106 1.697 8.483 42.104 5 1.030 5.150 49.256 1.030 5.150 49.256 1.430 7.152 49.256 6 .957 4.786 54.042 7 .907 4.535 58.577 8 .875 4.375 62.952 9 .863 4.317 67.268 10 .800 4.000 71.268 11 .718 3.592 74.860 12 .698 3.491 78.352 13 .659 3.296 81.647 14 .630 3.150 84.797 15 .580 2.899 87.696 16 .567 2.833 . 90.530 17 .536 2.678 93.207 18 .495 2.477 95.684 19 .479 2.394 98.078 20 .384 l .922 100.000 Extraction Method: Principal Component Analysis 175 Components Components 1 2 3 4 5 Q22N .638 .191 -5.152E-02 .286 -5.921E-02 Q22M .61] 4.078E-02 -9.367E-02 .130 1.779E-02 Q221 .591 .155 -6.776E-02 -.170 6.879E-02 Q22E .516 .105 -.173 -.252 .117 Q22R .501 .440 -.162 -.204 -5.982E-02 Q221 .499 .480 -3.l33E-02 -.149 -.161 Q22C 4.296E-02 .668 9.287E-02 -3.346E-02 .172 Q22r .163 .535 -.211 -8.148E-02 -5.460E-02 Q2213 .428 .523 -2.892E-02 5.419E-02 -5.974E-02 Q22F .104 .513 -.451 -.124 .220 Q22D .416 .450 -4.083E-02 2.538E-02 .231 szs -1.] l6E-02 —.128 .815 -2.260E—02 .110 Q22Q -.235 4.855E—03 .609 .120 30141302 Q22H .105 ‘1°%;5E' 4.034E-02 .78] -5.241E-02 Q22L .270 -.228 .341 .500 .239 Q22P -.202 -.147 -.100 .449 .336 Q22K -.295 '2'%6225' .339 .448 -2.983E-02 Q220 -.177 9.259E-02 .112 .115 .684 Q22A .263 .104 2.184E-02 -4.672E-02 .659 Q22G_RC .292 -.378 -.366 -.351 .390 Extraction Method: Principal Component Analysis Rotation Method: Varimax with Kaiser Normalization A Rotation converged in 9 iterations. Normal analysis requested Items have been assigned to factors as follows: 501 =4 3 21 502:6958 2] 503:101 31] Average correlation within clusters: .2683333 .252 .205 Standard score coefficient alphas: .595 .627 .508 Three Factor Analyses 176 Item and factor correlation matrix GJU'IWOI—‘NL’JD \I 10 12 13 11 501 502 503 4 22 34 24 17 13 17 33 11 34 16 12 ~6 ~13 47 43 3 2 1 34 24 17 32 25 26 25 30 35 26 35 24 10 16 12 19 14 18 25 27 28 15 27 28 20 24 23 O ~4 -6 7 0 ~6 ~6 ~12 ~15 ~9 ~17 ~26 56 55 49 35 43 43 ~4 ~18 ~29 6 13 10 16 12 22 19 31 20 26 -2 3 -12 -2 25 47 -7 9 17 19 14 18 l9 19 27 27 18 ~4 ~14 ~11 ~22 33 44 ~28 5 33 25 27 28 31 27 34 26 28 5 ~2 ~16 ~15 54 58 ~15 8 ll 15 27 28 20 27 26 27 30 ~12 ~17 ~9 ~22 39 52 ~33 ******* Sampling error analysis ******* Sample size = 608 Analysis for cluster number 1 Internal consistency analysis Within cluster correlation matrix for this cluster I-‘Nleb 501 4 22 34 24 17 47 3 34 32 25 26 56 2 24 25 30 35 55 1 17 26 35 24 49 Significance test for deviation from a flat within cluster correlation matrix. That is, a test 7 34 20 24 23 26 18 28 30 26 ~l 3 ~9 ~17 49 51 ~13 for the compound hypothesis (a) the items are unidimensional (all measure the same construct) and (b) the items are uniform in quality. The value of chisquare is 25.677 The degrees of freedom are 5 The tail probability is .000 10 16 O ~4 ~6 ~2 ~4 5 ~12 —l 33 34 15 23 3 ~6 58 Significance test for deviation from a unidimensional within cluster correlation matrix that allows for variation 12 12 7 O ~6 3 ~14 ~2 ~17 3 34 27 14 20 6 ~11 52 in quality. That is, a test of the hypothesis the items all measure the same construct which allows for a gradient in item quality. 177 13 ~6 ~6 ~12 ~15 ~12 ~11 ~16 ~9 ~9 15 14 9 17 ~19 ~23 30 11 501 502 ~13 47 43 ~9 56 35 ~17 55 43 ~26 49 43 ~2 25 47 ~22 33 44 ~15 54 58 ~22 39 52 ~17 49 51 23 3 ~6 20 6 ~11 17 ~19 ~23 18 ~31 ~31 ~31 100 79 ~31 79 100 43 ~22 ~38 503 ~4 ~18 ~29 ~7 ~28 ~15 ~33 ~13 58 52 3O 43 ~22 ~38 100 The value of chisquare is 22.822 The degrees of freedom are 5 The tail probability is .000 Analysis of parallelism Item by factor correlation matrix for this cluster 4 3 2 1 501 *** 502 43 35 43 43 503 5 ~4 ~18 ~29 Qualities ~- 501 47 56 55 49 Tests for parallelism Test for flat tests for parallel AND uniform quality Test with gradient allows for variation in quality Test for flat Test with g Cluster Chisq DF p Chisq 501 *** 502 2.317 3 0.509 7.390 503 28.235 3 0.000 26.938 TOTAL 37.164 6 0.000 41.758 Analysis for cluster number 2 lntemal consistency analysis Within cluster correlation matrix for this cluster 6 9 5 8 7 6 22 19 31 20 26 9 19 19 27 27 18 5 31 27 34 26 28 8 20 27 26 27 30 7 26 18 28 30 26 502 47 44 58 52 51 Significance test for deviation fiom a flat within cluster correlation matrix. That is, a test for the compound hypothesis (a) the items are unidimensional (all measure the same construct) and (b) the items are uniform in quality. 178 radient DF p 3 0.060 3 0.000 6 0.000 The value of chisquare is 20.536 The degrees of freedom are 9 The tail probability is .015 Significance test for deviation from a unidimensional within cluster correlation matrix that allows for variation in quality. That is, a test of the hypothesis the items all measure the same construct which allows for a gradient in item quality. The value of chisquare is 12.122 The degrees of freedom are 9 The tail probability is .207 Analysis of parallelism Item by factor correlation matrix for this cluster 6 9 5 8 7 501 25 33 54 39 49 502 *** 503 —7 ~28 ~15 ~33 ~13 Qualities ~— 502 47 44 58 52 51 Tests for parallelism Test for flat tests for parallel AND uniform quality Test with gradient allows for variation in quality Test for flat Test with gradient Cluster Chisq DF p Chisq DF p 501 27.784 4 0.000 15.093 4 0.005 502 *** 503 18.951 4 0.000 20.467 4 0.000 TOTAL 52.496 8 0.000 39.944 8 0.000 Analysis for cluster number 3 Internal consistency analysis Within cluster correlation matrix for this cluster 10 12 13 11 10 33 34 15 23 12 34 27 14 20 13 15 14 9 17 11 23 20 17 18 503 58 52 30 43 179 Significance test for deviation from a flat within cluster correlation matrix. That is, a test for the compound hypothesis (a) the items are unidimensional (all measure the same construct) and (b) the items are uniform in quality. The value of chisquare is 26.224 The degrees of freedom are 5 The tail probability is .000 Significance test for deviation from a unidimensional within cluster correlation matrix that allows for variation in quality. That is, a test of the hypothesis the items all measure the same construct which allows for a gradient in item quality. The value of chisquare is 5.084 The degrees of freedom are 5 The tail probability is .406 Analysis of parallelism Item by factor correlation matrix for this cluster 10 12 13 11 501 3 6 ~19 ~31 502 ~6 ~11 ~23 ~31 503 *** Qualities ~- 503 58 52 30 43 Tests for parallelism Test for flat tests for parallel AND uniform quality Test with gradient allows for variation in quality Test for flat Test with gradient Cluster Chisq DF p Chisq DF p 501 43.745 3 0.000 56.166 3 0.000 502 19.076 3 0.000 35.238 3 0.000 503 *** TOTAL 32.338 6 0.000 47.051 6 0.000 180 Two factor Analysis Normal analysis requested Items have been assigned to factors as follows: 501:432169587 502:10121311 Average correlation within clusters: .2297222 .205 Standard score coefficient alphas: .729 .508 Item and factor correlation matrix oomxooxr—‘ch. \J 10 12 13 11 501 502 4 23 34 24 17 13 17 33 11 34 16 12 ~6 ~13 48 5 3 2 1 34 24 17 20 25 26 25 25 35 26 35 24 10 16 12 19 14 18 25 27 28 15 27 28 20 24 23 0 ~4 ~6 7 0 ~6 ~6 ~12 ~15 ~9 ~17 ~26 45 50 49 ~4 ~18 ~29 6 13 10 16 12 14 19 31 20 26 ~2 3 ~12 ~2 37 ~7 9 17 19 14 18 19 17 27 27 18 ~4 ~14 ~11 ~22 41 ~28 5 33 25 27 28 31 27 37 26 28 5 ~2 ~16 ~15 61 ~15 8 11 15 27 28 20 27 26 23 30 ~12 ~17 ~9 -22 - 48 -33 _ ******* Sampling error analysis ******* Sample size = 608 Analysis for cluster number 1 Internal consistency analysis Within cluster correlation matrix for this cluster \ImU'IWOTP-‘NUJb 501 4 23 34 24 17 13 17 33 11 34 48 3 34 20 25 26 10 19 25 15 20 45 2 24 25 25 35 16 14 27 27 24 50 1 17 26 35 24 12 18 28 28 23 49 6 13 10 16 12 14 19 31 20 26 37 9 17 19 14 18 19 17 27 27 18 41 5 33 25 27 28 31 27 37 26 28 61 8 11 15 27 28 20 27 26 23 30 48 181 7 34 20 24 23 26 18 28 30 29 ~1 3 ~9 17 54 13 7 34 20 24 23 26 18 28 30 29 54 10 16 ~4 ~6 ~2 ~4 ~12 ~l 33 34 15 23 ~2 58 12 12 ~6 ~14 ~2 ~17 34 27 14 20 ~3 52 13 ~6 ~6 ~12 ~15 ~12 ~11 ~16 ~9 ~9 15 14 17 ~22 30 11 ~13 ~9 ~17 ~26 ~2 ~22 ~15 ~22 ~17 23 20 17 18 ~33 43 501 48 45 50 49 37 41 61 48 54 ~2 ~3 ~22 ~33 100 -33 502 ~4 ~18 ~29 ~7 ~28 ~15 ~33 ~13 58 52 30 43 -33 100 Significance test for deviation from a flat within cluster correlation matrix. That is, a test for the compound hypothesis (a) the items are unidimensional (all measure the same construct) and (b) the items are uniform in quality. The value of chisquare is 171.075 The degrees of freedom are 35 The tail probability is .000 Significance test for deviation from a unidimensional within cluster correlation matrix that allows for variation in quality. That is, a test of the hypothesis the items all measure the same construct which allows for a gradient in item quality. The value of chisquare is 113.144 The degrees of freedom are 35 The tail probability is .000 Analysis of parallelism Item by factor correlation matrix for this cluster 4 3 2 1 6 9 5 8 7 501 *** 502 5 ~4 ~18 ~29 ~7 ~28 ~15 ~33 ~13 Qualities ~— 501 48 45 50 49 37 41 61 48 54 Tests for parallelism Test for flat tests for parallel AND uniform quality Test with gradient allows for variation in quality Test for flat Test with gradient Cluster Chisq DF p Chisq DF p 501 *** 502 50.407 8 0.000 50.359 8 0.000 TOTAL 50.407 8 0.000 50.359 8 0.000 Analysis for cluster number 2 lntemal consistency analysis 182 Within cluster correlation matrix for this cluster 10 12 13 11 10 33 34 15 23 12 34 27 14 20 13 15 14 9 17 11 23 20 17 18 502 58 52 30 43 Significance test for deviation from a flat within cluster correlation matrix. That is, a test for the compound hypothesis (a) the items are unidimensional (all measure the same construct) and (b) the items are uniform in quality. The value of chisquare is 26.224 The degrees of freedom are 5 The tail probability is .000 Significance test for deviation from a unidimensional within cluster correlation matrix that allows for variation in quality. That is, a test of the hypothesis the items all measure the same construct which allows for a gradient in item quality. The value of chisquare is 5.084 The degrees of freedom are 5 The tail probability is .406 Analysis of parallelism Item by factor correlation matrix for this cluster 10 12 13 11 501 ~2 ~3 ~22 ~33 502 *** Qualities -~ 502 58 52 30 43 Tests for parallelism Test for flat tests for parallel AND uniform quality Test with gradient allows for variation in quality Test for flat Test with gradient Cluster Chisq DF p Chisq DF p 501 38.462 3 0.000 58.311 3 0.000 502 *** TOTAL 38.462 3 0.000 58.311 3 0.000 183 Appendix D: Cluster Analysis Output 184 Initial Cluster Centers Cluster 1 2 [FACTOR] 2.78 4.00 FACTOR3 5.00 .33 Iteration History Change in Cluster Centers Iteration 1 2 1 1.972 1.934 2 2.067E-02 2.437E-02 3 3.495E-03 4.106E-03 4 .000 .000 a Convergence achieved due to no or small distance change. The maximum distance by which any center has changed is .000. The current iteration is 4. The minimum distance between initial centers is 4.824. Final Cluster Centers Cluster 1 2 [FACTOR] 3.82 4.02 [FACTOR3 3.30 2.25 Distances between Final Cluster Centers Cluster 1 2 l 1 .075 2 1 .075 ANOVA Cluster Error F Sig. Mean df Mean df Square Square FACTORI 6.290 1 .241 597 26.060 .000 FACTOR3 165.871 1 .169 597 979.479 .000 The F tests should be used only for descriptive purposes because the clusters have been chosen to maximize the differences among cases in different clusters. The observed significance levels are not corrected for this and thus cannot be interpreted as tests of the hypothesis that the cluster means are equal. 185 Number of Cases in each Cluster Cluster 1 320.000 2 279.000 Valid 599.000 Missing 9.000 Group Statistics Cluster N Mean Std. Std. Error Members Deviation Mean hip FACTOR] 1 320 3.8153 .5221] .02919 2 279 4.0207 .45336 .02714 FACTOR3 1 320 3.3010 .40889 .02286 2 279 2.2461 .4145] .02482 Independent Samples Test Levene's Test for Equality t-test for of Equality of Variances Means F Sig. t df Sig. Mean Std. Error 95% (2- iDifl‘erencelDifl‘erence Confidence tailed) Interval of the Difference Lower Upper IFACTORllEqual 8.859.003 -5.105 597 .000 -.2054 .04024 -.2845 -.12640 variancesl lassumed ual -5.154596.991.000 -.2054 .03986 -.2837-.12715 ariancesw 0t sumed 1FACTOR3 qua] 2.067.]5131297 597 .000 1.0549 .0337] .9887 1.12112 Eariancesr sumed ual 31267583678 .000 1.0549 .03374.988661.12119 ariancesr 0t sumed 186 Appendix E: Fairness Heuristic Analysis Output 187 Statistics N Mean Std. Deviation Valid Missing Q22A 597 l 1 4.08 .655 Q22E 600 8 3.99 .670 Q22C 593 15 3.50 .976 Q22D 598 10 3 .96 .805 Q22E 600 8 4.39 .680 Q22F 593 15 3.98 .869 Q226_RC 596 12 3.96 .874 Q22H 586 22 3.05 .943 Q221 592 16 3.84 .766 Q22] 596 12 4.35 .717 Q22K 597 1] 1.94 .910 Q22L 595 13 3.53 .851 Q22M 588 20 4.05 .77] Q22N 586 22 3.85 .661 Q220 585 23 3.19 .844 Q22P 584 24 2.67 .985 Q22Q 578 30 2.3] .892 Q22R 590 18 3.97 .825 Q2ZS 588 20 2.59 .957 Q22T 586 22 3.53 .952 188 Frequencies Strongly Strongly Total Mean Std. Disagree Disagree Neutral Agree Agree Dev. (1) (2) (3) (4) (5) Q22a Frequency 1 14 59 388 135 597 4 .08 0.655 Valid Percent 0.2 2.3 9.9 65 22.6 100 Q22h Frequency 6 12 65 4] 5 102 600 3 .99 0.67 Valid Percent l 2 10.8 69.2 17 100 Q22e Frequency 1 7 81 I60 259 76 593 3.5 0.976 Valid Percent 2.9 13.7 27 43.7 12.8 100 Q22d Frequency 6 32 73 354 1 33 598 3.96 0.805 Valid Percent 1 5.4 12.2 59.2 22.2 100 Q22e Frequency 0 8 43 255 294 600 4.39 0.68 Valid Percent 0 1.3 7.2 42.5 49 100 Q22f Frequency 7 30 97 292 167 593 3 .98 0.869 Valid Percent 1.2 5.1 16.4 49.2 28.2 100 QZZg—RC Frequency 12 28 82 32] 153 596 3.96 0.874 Valid Percent 2 4.7 13.8 53.9 25.7 100 Q22h Frequency 30 130 229 l 72 25 586 3.05 0.943 Valid Percent 5.1 22.2 39.1 29.4 4.3 100 Q22i Frequency 4 26 l 25 341 96 592 3 .84 0.766 Valid Percent 0.7 4.4 21.1 57.6 16.2 100 Q22j Frequeng 8 5 22 296 265 596 4.35 0.717 Valid Percent 1.3 0.8 3.7 49.7 44.5 100 922k Frequency 201 280 75 30 l 1 597 1.94 0.91 Valid Percent 33.7 46.9 12.6 5 1.8 100 Q22I Frequency 8 67 174 296 50 595 3.53 0.85] Valid Percent 1.3 11.3 29.2 49.7 8.4 100 QZZm Frequency 7 2] 56 357 147 588 4.05 0.77] Valid Percent 1.2 3.6 9.5 60.7 25 100 Q22n Frequency 3 24 87 414 58 586 3.85 0.66] Valid Percent 0.5 4.1 14.8 70.6 9.9 100 Q220 Frequency 16 91 264 191 23 585 3.19 0.844 Valid Percent 2.7 15.6 45.1 32.6 3.9 100 Q22p Frequency 59 21 1 196 97 21 584 2 .67 0.985 Valid Percent 10.] 36.1 33.6 16.6 3.6 100 Q221 Frequency 85 306 120 59 8 578 2.3] 0.893 Valid Percent 14.7 52.9 20.8 10.2 1.4 100 Q22r Frequency 13 15 86 338 138 590 3.97 0.825 Valid Percent 2.2 2.5 14.6 57.3 23.4 100 0223 Frequency 61 240 178 95 I4 588 2 .59 0.957 Valid Percent 10.4 40.8 30.3 16.2 2.4 100 Q22t Frequency 14 7] I69 253 79 586 3.53 0.952 Valid Percent 2.4 12.1 28.8 43.2 13.5 100 189 Agree and Strongly Agree Combined Responses for Fairness Heuristic Levene's Test for Equality of t-test for Equality of Means Variances . Si . F Sig. t (11' (2-taigl ed) Q22AREDU Equal variances assumed 13.394 .000 -1.828 594 .068 Equal variances not assumed -l.860 587.395 .063 Q22BREDU Equal variances assumed 44.159 .000 -3.372 596 .00] Equal variances not assumed -3.450 575.430 .00] Q22CREDU Equal variances assumed .061 .805 -1.318 591 .188 Equal variances not assumed -I .318 579.206 .188 Q22DREDU Equal variances assumed 10.858 .001 -1.813 595 .070 Equal variances not assumed -1.829 594.931 .068 Q22EREDU Equal variances assumed 25.398 .000 -2.535 597 .01] Equal variances not assumed -2.587 581.198 .010 Q22PREDU Equal variances assumed 72.61 I .000 -4.751 591 .000 Equal variances not assumed -4.847 576.795 .000 QZZGREDU Equal variances assumed 83.254 .000 -4.638 594 .000 Egal variances not assumed -4.774 558.056 .000 Q22HREDU Equal variances assumed 2.290 .131 21.486 584 .000 Era] variances not assumed 21.360 551.218 .000 Q221REDU Equal variances assumed 55.509 .000 -4.560 590 .000 Equal variances not assumed -4.628 588.347 .000 QZZJREDU Equal variances assumed .247 .619 -.303 593 .762 Egral variances not assumed -.301 564.008 .764 Q22KREDU Equal variances assumed 700.414 .000 10.519 595 .000 Equal variances not assumed I 1.252 344.719 .000 Q22LREDU Equal variances assumed 68.581 .000 15.813 593 .000 Eflal variances not assumed 15.207 418.055 .000 Q22MREDU Equal variances assumed 2.317 .128 -.903 583 .367 Equal variances not assumed -.904 576.633 .366 QQZNREDU Equal variances assumed 3.057 .081 -.793 581 .428 Equal variances not assumed -.799 580.997 .424 Q220REDU Equal variances assumed 16.214 .000 2.369 580 .018 Equal variances not assumed 2.384 578.922 .017 Q22PREDU Equal variances assumed .365 .546 3.887 579 .000 Equal variances not assumed 3.904 577.871 .000 QZQREDU Equal variances assumed 55.698 .000 5.241 573 .000 Equal variances not assumed 5.316 565.610 .000 Q22RREDU Equal variances assumed 63.481 .000 -4.300 583 .000 Equal variances not assumed -4.374 566.935 .000 Q22SREDU Equal variances assumed 7.370 .007 3.954 583 .000 Equal variances not assumed 3.984 582.940 .000 Q22TREDU Equal variances assumed 15.049 .000 -3.604 581 .000 Equal variances not assumed -3.636 580.465 .000 190 — ~.o H QO§E> .WV— .9 H HOHONM huemo S #0: nab: MO :50: WOZRQAU OH. m—UZ/th—mmd H mac: BN8?“ mgukémms: 302 H mp5: “gov—om _ sown—r :VOM. 2b.- Inna—r 86.- cows. snmafi enema». 9.9—N.- .I6NV~. toga. wmQr m8: nevhN. onhhq. .551 saamfi oaNhN. «N8. «N60 — 21.2.. usmmm. one. uno:. owner #8... :mmm. :OON. .uon:.r comic mmo. «soc—r suhwmr a-Nzr «cam: 68.-. v8.» hmo. mNNo — .va—r now—_r mvor Ivnwn. DISN. :or IQNONI 30m... rochv. _wor nomO. 0J~ON. OOVNW. IQMOm. :No—. .TJOQM. nos. uNNO — sacs. one. N59. cor—NF 6:2. :OVN. .13—r :ooN; :mfl. son—N; :3”- rim—... ooh—u: 03.- .652: 35.. UNNO « .... ..8..__..§..u a... £2. ......Ea , . as, was ...:q-.. ..VE.........s.s. . as .. .31.... was a... , as. _ mm... .. 86.- :5. MB. .87 .28. :o. «8. 8a.- 3a.. .62. 68.- :62. 38 — :mvm. :VN — . :cN _ r :ONM. a. v2. n8.. lama: . Inv:. :- wvm. :wN— . noonm. cnnz. ENNO n nha. omgr Ion—u. onwV—. . QIOWN. CIOON. 008. comma. nomuu. INNO , . . _ . , as. ......33; ,_ . 8. ..aa . . .....s as. _ as .. . . .61.- . . a, —. ..63; 3.3..» y _ .32. ..E. [.83. .62. 38 o—or GOOBN. Gabon. §._m_. _NNO .....E... p.33 .... «.35.........aq. ...H 3.8 ._ . _ 8:. . «as. . :32. 91.38 _ .33. .62. :8 coth. lummfi DNNO «icwm. GIFVN. :8 — semen. «m8. 0N8 — o I — o— . has 38 _ .8 38 38 £8 .38 6N8 .38 330 R8 38 :8 a8 .38 8.1338 :8 £8 £8 38 .38 38 9:3— 0.323: 32:25 .5952. 25322.50 191 (122A Frequency Percent Valid Percent Cumulative % Valid Sirong‘y l .2 .2 .2 Drsagree Disagree 14 2.3 2.3 2.5 Neutral 59 9.7 9.9 12.4 Agree 388 63.8 65.0 77.4 Strongly Agree 135 22.2 22.6 100.0 Total 597 98.2 100.0 Missing System 1 l 1.8 Total 608 100.0 QZZB Frequency Percent Valid Percent Cumulative % Valid 330”” 6 1.0 1.0 1.0 Drsagree Disagree 12 2.0 2.0 3.0 Neutral 65 10.7 10.8 13.8 Agree 415 68.3 69.2 83.0 Strongly Agree 102 16.8 17.0 100.0 Total 600 98.7 100.0 Missing System 8 1.3 Total 608 100.0 22C Frequency Percent Valid Percent Cumulative % Valid 330”” 17 2.8 2.9 2.9 Drsagree Disagree 8] 13.3 13.7 16.5 Neutral 160 26.3 27.0 43.5 Agree 259 42.6 43.7 87.2 SW‘gly 76 12.5 12.8 100.0 Agree Total 593 97.5 100.0 Missing System 15 2.5 Total 608 100.0 22D Frequency Percent Valid Percent Cumulative % Valid Sirong'y 6 1.0 1.0 1.0 Drsagree Disagree 32 5.3 5.4 6.4 Neutral 73 12.0 12.2 18.6 Agree 354 58.2 59.2 77.8 Strongly Agree 133 21.9 22.2 100.0 Total 598 98.4 100.0 Missing System 10 1.6 Total 608 100.0 192 IQ22E Frequeng Percent Valid Percent Cumulative % Valid Disagree 8 1 .3 1.3 1 .3 Neutral 43 7.] 7.2 8.5 Agree 255 41.9 42.5 51.0 Strongly Agree 294 48.4 49.0 100.0 Total 600 98.7 100.0 Missing System 8 l .3 Total 608 100.0 Q22F Frequency Percent Valid Percent Cumulative % Valid Simng‘y 7 1.2 1.2 1.2 Drsagree Disagree 30 4.9 5.1 6.2 Neutral 97 16.0 16.4 22.6 Agree 292 48.0 49.2 71.8 Strongly Agree 167 27.5 28.2 100.0 Total 593 97.5 100.0 Missing System 15 2.5 Total 608 100.0 Q226_RC Frequency Percent Valid Percent ICumulative % Valid Sirong'y 12 2.0 2.0 2.0 Drsagree Disagree 28 4.6 4.7 6.7 Neutral 82 13.5 13.8 20.5 Agree 32] 52.8 53.9 74.3 Strongly Agree 153 25.2 25.7 100.0 Total 596 98.0 100.0 Missing System 12 2.0 Total 608 100.0 22H Frequency Percent Valid Percent Cumulative % Valid Simngly 30 4.9 5.1 5.1 Drsagree Disagree 130 21.4 22.2 27.3 Neutral 229 37.7 39.] 66.4 Agree 172 28.3 29.4 95.7 Strongly Agree 25 4.1 4.3 100.0 Total 586 96.4 100.0 Missing System 22 3.6 Total 608 100.0 Q22] Frequency Percent Valid Percent Cumulative % Valid Sirong'y 4 .7 .7 .7 Drsagree Disagree 26 4. 3 4.4 5.] Neutral 125 20.6 21.] 26.2 Agree 34] 56.1 57.6 83.8 Strongly Agree 96 15.8 16.2 100.0 Total 592 97.4 100.0 Missing System 16 2.6 Total 608 100.0 Q22J Frequency Percent Valid Percent Cumulative % Valid Sirong‘y 8 1.3 1.3 1.3 Drsagree Disagree 5 .8 .8 2.2 Neutral 22 3.6 3.7 5.9 Agree 296 48.7 49.7 55.5 Strongly Agree 265 43.6 44.5 100.0 Total 596 98.0 100.0 Missing System 12 2.0 Total 608 100.0 22K Frequency Percent Valid Percent Cumulative % Valid Si’°“g'y 201 33.1 33.7 33.7 Drsagree Disagree 280 46.1 46.9 80.6 Neutral 75 12.3 12.6 93.] Agree 30 4.9 5.0 98.2 Strongly Agree 11 1.8 1.8 100.0 Total 597 98.2 100.0 Missing System 1 1 1.8 Total 608 100.0 [622L FrecRency Percent Valid Percent Cumulative % Valid girong‘y 8 1.3 1.3 1.3 lsagree Disagree 67 11.0 11.3 12.6 Neutral 174 28.6 29.2 41.8 Agree 296 48.7 49.7 91.6 Strongly Agree 50 8.2 8.4 100.0 Total 595 97.9 100.0 Missing System 13 2.1 Total 608 100.0 194 Q22M Frequency Percent Valid Percent Cumulative % Valid Sirong'y 7 1.2 1.2 1.2 Drsagree Disagree 2] 3.5 3.6 4.8 Neutral 56 9.2 9.5 14.3 Agree 357 58.7 60.7 75.0 Strongly Agree 147 24.2 25.0 100.0 Total 588 96.7 100.0 Missing System 20 3.3 Total 608 100.0 Q22N Frequency Percent Valid Percent Cumulative % Valid 330“!” 3 .5 .5 .5 Drsagee Diszquee 24 3.9 4.] 4.6 Neutral 87 14.3 14.8 19.5 Agree 414 68.1 70.6 90.] Strongly Agee 58 9.5 9.9 100.0 Total 586 96.4 100.0 Missing System 22 3.6 Total 608 100.0 [Q220 Frequency Percent Valid Percent Cumulative % Valid 53°“g'y 16 2.6 2.7 2.7 Drsagree Disagree 9] 15.0 15.6 18.3 Neutral 264 43.4 45.1 63.4 Agree 19] 31.4 32.6 96.1 Strongly Agree 23 3.8 3.9 100.0 Total 585 96.2 100.0 Missing System 23 3.8 Total 608 100.0 R222P Frequency Percent Valid Percent Cumulative % Valid 580%” 59 9.7 10.1 10.1 Drsagree Disagree 2] 1 34.7 36.1 46.2 Neutral 196 32.2 33.6 79.8 Agree 97 16.0 16.6 96.4 Strongly Agree 21 3.5 3.6 100.0 Total 584 96.1 100.0 Missing System 24 3.9 Total 608 100.0 195 |Q22Q Frequency Percent Valid Percent lCumulative % Valid Sirongly 85 14.0 14.7 14.7 Drsagree Disagree 306 50.3 52.9 67.6 Neutral 120 19.7 20.8 88.4 Agree 59 9.7 10.2 98.6 Strongly Agree 8 1.3 1.4 100.0 Total 578 95.1 100.0 Missing System 30 4.9 Total 608 100.0 [Q22R Frequency Percent Valid Percent Cumulative % Valid 5‘30“” 13 2.1 2.2 2.2 Drsagree Disagree 15 2.5 2.5 4.7 Neutral 86 14.] 14.6 19.3 Agree 338 55.6 57.3 76.6 Strongly Agree 138 22.7 23.4 100.0 Total 590 97.0 100.0 Missing System 18 3 .0 Total 608 100.0 |Q228 Frequency Percent Valid Percent Cumulative % Valid 35mg" 61 10.0 10.4 10.4 Drsagree Disagree 240 39.5 40.8 5 1.2 Neutral 178 29.3 30.3 81.5 Agree 95 15.6 16.2 97.6 Strongly Agree 14 2.3 2.4 100.0 Total 588 96.7 100.0 Missigg System 20 3.3 Total 608 100.0 Q22T Frequency Percent Valid Percent Cumulative % Valid Simng‘y 14 2.3 2.4 2.4 Drsagree Disagree 7] 11.7 12.] 14.5 Neutral 169 27.8 28.8 43.3 Agree 253 41.6 43.2 86.5 Strongly Agree 79 13.0 13.5 100.0 Total 586 96.4 100.0 Missing System 22 3.6 Total 608 100.0 Q22AREDU Total 2.00 3.00 4.00 cm“ . 1 Count 11 35 273 319 embershrp % Within Cluster 3.4% 1 1.0% 85.6% 100.0% 'Membership 2 Count 4 24 249 277 % Within Cluster 1.4% 8.7% 89.9% 100.0% LMemberslmr Total Count 15 59 y 522 596 % Within Cluster 2.5% 9.9% 87.6% 100.0% [Membership Chi-Square Tests Value df Asymp. Sig. (2-sided) Pearson Chi-Sgare 3.479 2 .176 Likelihood Ratio 3.602 2 .165 L‘“ear'b¥'l:‘“ea' 3.329 1 .068 Assocratron N of Valid Cases 596 0 cells (.0%) have expected count less than 5. The minimum expected count is 6.97. Q22BREDU Total 2.00 3.00 4.00 1114311133111 1 Count 12 47 261 320 P % Within Cluster 3.8% 14.7% 81.6% 100.0% Membership 2 Count 5 18 255 278 % Within Cluster 1.8% 6.5% 91.7% 100.0% Membershi Total Count 1 7 65 5 16 598 % Within Cluster 2.8% 10.9% 86.3% 100.0% Membership Chi-Square Tests Value df Asymp. Sig. (2-sided Pearson Chi-Square 13.005 2 .001 Likelihood Ratio 13.493 2 .001 L‘“ea"b.y“:‘“ea’ 1 1.179 1 .001 Assocratlon N of Valid Cases 598 0 cells (.0%) have expected count less than 5. The minimum expected count is 7.90. 197 Q22CREDU Total 2.00 3.00 4.00 cm” . 1 Count 54 95 169 318 embershlp % Within Cluster 17.0% 29.9% 53.1% 100.0% [Membership 2 Count 44 65 166 275 % Within Cluster 16.0% 23.6% 60.4% 100.0% embership Total Count 98 160 335 593 % Within Cluster 16.5% 27.0% 56.5% 100.0% [Membership Chi-Square Tests Value df Asymp. Sig. (2-sided)1 Pearson Chi-Square 3.573 2 .168 Likelihood Ratio 3.587 2 .166 Ll’far'bY'Ii‘nea’ 1.735 1 .188 ssoclatlon N of Valid Cases 593 0 cells (.0%) have expected count less than 5. The minimum expected count is 45.45. Q22DREDU Total 2.00 3.00 4.00 Meflgigip 1 Count 23 46 250 319 % Within Cluster 7.2% 14.4% 78.4% 100.0% LMembership 2 Count 15 27 236 278 % Within Cluster 5.4% 9.7% 84.9% 100.0% Membership Total Count 38 73 486 597 % Within Cluster 6.4% 12.2% 81.4% 100.0% [Membership Chi-Square Tests Value df Asymp. Sig. (2-sided) Pearson Chi-Square 4.237 2 .120 Likelihood Ratio 4.285 2 .117 Li’far'bY'Irinea’ 3.276 1 .070 ssocratlon N of Valid Cases 597 0 cells (.0%) have expected count less than 5. The minimum expected count is 17.70. 198 Q22EREDU Total 2.00 3.00 4.00 M62321“) 1 Count 5 32 283 320 % Within Cluster 1.6% 10.0% 88.4% 100.0% 'Membership 2 Count 3 1 1 265 279 % Within Cluster 1.1% 3.9% 95.0% 100.0% 'Membership Total Count 8 43 548 599 % Within Cluster 1.3% 7.2% 91.5% 100.0% [Membership Chi-Square Tests Value df Asymp. Sig. (2-sided) Pearson Chi-Sgare 8.581 2 .014 Likelihood Ratio 8.996 2 .01] Linear'bY'Itinea’ 6.370 1 .012 Assocratlon N of Valid Cases 599 0 cells (.0%) have expected count less than 5. The minimum expected count is 3.73. QZZFREDU Total 2.00 3.00 4.00 mum . 1 Count 26 72 219 317 embershlp % Within Cluster 8.2% 22.7% 69.1% 100.0% 'Membership 2 Count 11 25 240 276 % Within Cluster 4.0% 9.1% 87.0% 100.0% 'Membership Total Count 37 97 459 593 % Within Cluster 6.2% 16.4% 77.4% 100.0% [Membership Chi-Square Tests Value df Asymp. Sig. (2-sided) Pearson Chi-Square 27.110 2 .000 Likelihood Ratio 28.144 2 .000 Law'bY'lfma’ 21.776 1 .000 ssocratlon N of Valid Cases 593 0 cells (.0%) have expected count less than 5. The minimum expected count is 17.22. 199 QZZGREDU Total 2.00 3.00 4.00 Meggjghip 1 Count 32 55 233 320 % Within Cluster 10.0% 17.2% 72.8% 100.0% [Membership 2 Count 27 241 276 % Within Cluster 2.9% 9.8% 87.3% 100.0% Membership Total Count 40 82 474 596 % Within Cluster 6.7% 13.8% 79.5% 100.0% [Membership Chi-Square Tests Value df Asymp. Sig. (2-sided) Pearson Chi-Square 20.962 2 .000 Likelihood Ratio 22.059 2 .000 me'bY'Ir‘m’ 20.795 1 .000 Assoclatlon N of Valid Cases 596 0 cells (.0%) have expected count less than 5. The minimum expected count is 18.52. Q22HREDU Total 2.00 3.00 4.00 11463232815 1 Count 1 1 126 182 319 % Within Cluster 3.4% 39.5% 57.1% 100.0% 'Membership 2 Count 149 103 15 267 % Within Cluster 55.8% 38.6% 5.6% 100.0% 'Membership Total Count 160 229 197 586 % Within Cluster 27.3% 39.1% 33.6% 100.0% [Membership Chi-Square Tests Value df Asymp. Sig. (2-sided Pearson Chi-Square 260.339 2 .000 Likelihood Ratio 306.392 2 .000 L‘Ikear'b¥"7‘“ea' 258.270 1 .000 ssoclatlon N of Valid Cases 586 0 cells (.0%) have expected count less than 5. The minimum expected count is 72.90. 200 Q221REDU Total 2.00 3.00 4.00 Chm . 1 Count 19 92 207 318 embershrp % Within Cluster 6.0% 28.9% 65.1% 100.0% LMembership 2 Count 1 1 33 230 274 % Within Cluster 4.0% 12.0% 83.9% 100.0% [Membership Total Count 30 125 437 592 % Within Cluster 5.1% 21.1% 73.8% 100.0% [Membership Chi-Square Tests Value df Asymp. Sig. (2-sided) Pearson Chi-Scplare 28.077 2 .000 Likelihood Ratio 29.084 2 .000 “‘far'bY'ITinear 20.1 19 1 .000 ssocratron N of Valid Cases 592 0 cells (.0%) have expected count less than 5. The minimum expected count is 13.89. Q221REDU Total 2.00 3.00 4.00 Clum . 1 Count 5 17 297 319 embershlp % Within Cluster 1.6% 5.3% 93.1% 100.0% embership 2 Count 8 5 263 276 % Within Cluster 2.9% 1.8% 95.3% 100.0% 'Membership Total Count 1 3 22 560 595 % Within Cluster 2.2% 3.7% 94.1% 100.0% [Membership Chi—Square Tests Value df Asymp. Sig. (2-sided) Pearson Chi-Square 6.227 2 .044 Likelihood Ratio 6.570 2 .037 Llaear'b¥"7mea’ .092 1 .762 ssocratron N of Valid Cases 595 0 cells (.0%) have expected count less than 5. The minimum expected count is 6.03. 201 Q22KREDU Total 2.00 3.00 4.00 L4 01‘8“” . 1 Count 209 70 41 320 embership % Within Cluster 65.3% 21.9% 12.8% 100.0% Membership 2 Count 272 5 277 % Within Cluster 98.2% 1.8% 100.0% Membership Total Count 48 l 75 4 l 597 % Within Cluster 80.6% 12.6% 6.9% 100.0% [Membership Chi-Square Tests Value df Asymp. Sig. (2-sided)1 Pearson Chi-Square 103.022 2 .000 Likelihood Ratio 129.246 2 .000 “‘fm'bflrma’ 93.456 1 .000 ssocratron N of Valid Cases 597 0 cells (.0%) have expected count less than 5. The minimum expected count is 19.02. 202 Q22LREDU Total 2.00 3.00 4.00 cm“ . 1 Count 4 52 264 320 embershlp % Within Cluster 1.3% 16.3% 82.5% 100.0% 'Membership 2 Count 71 122 82 275 % Within Cluster 25.8% 44.4% 29.8% 100.0% embership Total Count 75 174 346 595 % Within Cluster 12.6% 29.2% 58.2% 100.0% [Membership Chi-Square Tests Value df Asymp. Sig. (2-sided)l Pearson Chi-Square 181.382 2 .000 Likelihood Ratio 199.033 2 .000 ”‘far'bl/‘liinea' 176.182 1 .000 ssocratron N of Valid Cases 595 0 cells (.0%) have expected count less than 5. The minimum expected count is 34.66. Q22MREDU Total 2.00 3.00 4.00 mum" . 1 Count 14 37 260 31 1 embershrp % Within Cluster 4.5% 11.9% 83.6% 100.0% embership 2 Count 14 19 241 274 % Within Cluster 5.1% 6.9% 88.0% 100.0% 'Membership Total Count 28 56 50] 585 % Within Cluster 4.8% 9.6% 85.6% 100.0% [Membership Chi-Square Tests Value df Asymp. Sig. (2-sided) Pearson Chi-Square 4.183 2 .124 Likelihood Ratio 4.269 2 .118 Lfiw‘bl'lrmea’ .815 1 .367 ssocratron N of Valid Cases 585 0 cells (.0%) have expected count less than 5. The minimum expected count is 13.11. Q22NREDU Total 2.00 3.00 4.00 [M Clus'f‘ . 1 Count 18 44 248 310 embershrp % Within Cluster 5.8% 14.2% 80.0% 100.0% embership 2 Count 9 43 221 27 3 % Within Cluster 3.3% 15.8% 81.0% 100.0% lMembership Total Count 27 87 469 583 % Within Cluster 4.6% 14.9% 80.4% 100.0% [Membership Chi-Square Tests Value df Asymp. Sig. (2-sided Pearson Chi-Square 2.227 2 .328 Likelihood Ratio 2.275 2 .32] “’far'W'Ir‘ma’ .629 1 .428 ssocratron N of Valid Cases 583 0 cells (.0%) have expected count less than 5. The minimum expected count is 12.64. 203 Q220REDU Total 2.00 3.00 4.00 mum . 1 Count 57 120 134 311 embershrp % Within Cluster 18.3% 38.6% 43.1% 100.0% Membership 2 Count 49 144 78 27] % Within Cluster 18.1% 53.1% 28.8% 100.0% Membership Total Count 106 264 2 1 2 5 82 % Within Cluster 18.2% 45.4% 36.4% 100.0% [Membership Chi-Square Tests Value df Asymp. Sig. (2-sided) Pearson Chi-Square 14.899 2 .001 Likelihood Ratio 15.007 2 .001 ”riar‘bflfinear 5.567 1 .018 ssocratron N of Valid Cases 582 0 cells (.0%) have expected count less than 5. The minimum expected count is 49.36. Q22PREDU Total 2.00 3.00 4.00 mum . 1 Count 122 110 76 308 embershrp % Within Cluster 39.6% 35.7% 24.7% 100.0% Membership 2 Count 148 85 40 273 % Within Cluster 54.2% 31.1% 14.7% 100.0% Membership Total Count 270 195 1 16 581 % Within Cluster 46.5% 33.6% 20.0% 100.0% [Membership Chi-Square Tests Value df Asymp. Sig. (2-sided Pearson Chi-Square 14.827 2 .001 Likelihood Ratio 14.97] 2 .001 Lifar‘bY'lfinea’ 14.746 1 .000 ssocratron N of Valid Cases 581 0 cells (.0%) have expected count less than 5. The minimum expected count is 54.51. 204 Q22QREDU Total 2.00 3.00 4.00 cm” . 1 Count 176 82 47 305 embershrp % Within Cluster 57.7% 26.9% 15.4% 100.0% Membership 2 Count 213 38 19 270 % Within Cluster 78.9% 14.1% 7.0% 100.0% [Membership Total Count 389 120 66 575 % Within Cluster 67.7% 20.9% 11.5% 100.0% [Membership Chi-Square Tests Value df Asymp. Sig. (2-sided) Pearson Chi-Square 29.510 2 .000 Likelihood Ratio 30.172 2 .000 “near'bY'If‘near 26.253 1 .000 Assocratlon N of Valid Cases 575 0 cells (.0%) have expected count less than 5. The minimum expected count is 30.99. Q22RREDU Total 2.00 3.00 4.00 Clusw’ . 1 Count 18 65 227 310 embershrp % Within Cluster 5.8% 21.0% 73.2% 100.0% embership 2 Count 9 21 245 275 % Within Cluster 3.3% 7.6% 89.1% 100.0% embership Total Count 27 86 472 585 % Within Cluster 4.6% 14.7% 80.7% 100.0% [Membership Chi-Square Tests Value df Asymp. Sig. (2-sided Pearson Chi-Square 24.191 2 .000 Likelihood Ratio 25.263 2 .000 ”’far'bfgnea’ 17.950 1 .000 ssocratron N of Valid Cases 585 0 cells (.0%) have expected count less than 5. The minimum expected count is 12.69. 205 Q22SREDU Total 2.00 3.00 4.00 Mgrllbsetresrhip 1 Count 139 97 74 310 % Within Cluster 44.8% 31.3% 23.9% 100.0% embership 2 Count 160 81 34 275 % Within Cluster 58.2% 29.5% 12.4% 100.0% Membership Total Count 299 l 78 108 5 85 % Within Cluster 51.1% 30.4% 18.5% 100.0% [Membershi Chi-Square Tests Value df Asymp. Sig. (2-sided) Pearson Chi-Square 15.690 2 .000 Likelihood Ratio 15.995 2 .000 Linear‘bY’Itinea’ 15.249 1 .000 Assocratlon N of Valid Cases 585 0 cells (.0%) have expected count less than 5. The minimum expected count is 50.77. 206 Q22TREDU Total 7 2.00 3.00 4.00 mum . 1 Count 58 94 157 309 embershrp % Within Cluster 18.8% 30.4% 50.8% 100.0% Membership 2 Count 26 75 17 3 274 % Within Cluster 9.5% 27.4% 63.1% 100.0% Membership Total Count 84 169 330 5 83 % Within Cluster 14.4% 29.0% 56.6% 100.0% [Membership Chi-Square Tests Value df Asymp. Sig. (2-sided) Pearson Chi-Square 13.048 2 .001 Likelihood Ratio 13.318 2 .001 Li‘far'b¥'l:i“ea’ 12.725 1 .000 ssocratron N of Valid Cases 583 0 cells (.0%) have expected count less than 5. The minimum expected count is 39.48. Appendix F: BMP Analysis 207 BMP Results and Preferences Would Do PP° P90 PP0 “PP Strongly Know P 0208 Frequency 21 98 193 239 36 21 608 Valid Percent 3.5 16.1 31 .7 39.3 71 5.9 3.5 100 Q20b Frequency 53 163 216 1 18 39 19 608 Valid Percent 8.7 26.8 35.5 19.4 54.9 6.4 3.1 100 Q20e Frequency 1 6 65 238 24 l 30 l 8 608 Valid Percent 2.6 10.7 39.1 39.6 78.7 4.9 3 100 Q20d Frequency 10 42 151 376 l6 13 608 Valid Percent 1.6 6.9 24.8 61.8 86.6 2.6 2.1 100 Q20e Frequency 2 l 60 206 286 19 l 6 608 Valid Percent 3.5 9.9 33.9 47 80.9 3.1 2.6 100 Q20f Frequency 12 75 260 227 l 7 1 7 608 Valid Percent 2 12.3 42.8 37.3 80.1 2.8 2.8 100 Q20 Frequency 9 72 278 210 23 16 608 Valid Percent 1.5 l 1.8 45.7 34.5 80.2 3.8 2.6 100 Would Do Individualist Cluster “é?“ 1:” SMigh; SWoulgt Ssh-0mg and Not R No Total ppo uppo uppo uppo Strong] Know espouse Q20a Frequency 16 59 l 15 91 31 8 320 Valid Percent 5 18.4 35.9 28.4 64.3 9.7 2.5 100 Q20b Frequency 3 l 93 l 20 42 26 8 320 Valid Percent 9.7 29.1 37.5 13.1 50.6 8.1 2.5 100 Q20c Frequency 13 48 135 99 20 5 320 Valid Percent 4.1 15 42.2 30.9 73.1 6.3 1.6 100 Q20d Frequency 8 33 90 l 73 12 4 320 Valid Percent 2.5 10.3 28.1 54.1 82.2 3.8 1.3 100 Q20e Frequency 19 48 l 17 l 16 16 4 320 Valid Percent 5.9 15 36.6 36.3 72.9 5 1.3 100 Q20f Frequency 8 48 142 104 l 3 5 320 Valid Percent 2.5 15 44.4 32.5 76.9 4.1 1.6 100 Q20; Frequency 5 41 l 61 93 l 5 5 320 Valid Percent 1.6 12.8 50.3 29.1 79.4 4.7 1.6 100 208 Would . Would Do Egalitarian Cluster Not SMlgh; SWoultll1 :tronglly and Not R No Total Slpmn up po uppo "FPO Stron Iy Know esponse Q2 08 Frequency 5 38 78 148 4 6 279 Valid Percent 1.8 13.6 28 53 81 1.4 2.2 100 Q20b Frequency 22 69 96 76 l 3 3 279 Valid Percent 7.9 24.7 34.4 27.2 61.6 4.7 1.1 100 Q20e Frequency 2 l 7 l 03 l 42 9 6 279 Valid Percent 0.7 6.1 36.9 50.9 87.8 3.2 2.2 100 Q20d Frequency 2 8 61 202 4 2 2 79 Valid Percent 0.7 2.9 21.9 72.4 94.3 1.4 0.7 100 Q20e Frequency 1 l 2 89 l 70 3 4 279 Valid Percent 0.4 4.3 31.9 60.9 92.8 1.1 1.4 100 Q20f Frequency 4 26 l 18 123 4 4 279 Valid Percent 1.4 9.3 42.3 44.1 86.4 1.4 1.4 100 Q20g Frequency 4 30 1 l7 1 l7 7 4 279 Valid Percent 1.4 10.8 41.9 41.9 83.8 2.5 1.4 100 Independent Samples Test of BMP at .01 Level of Significance lLevene's Test for Equality o t-test for Equality of Means Variances 0 F Sig dr Sig' (2' Me“ S‘d‘ Em” 9:11:37??? ° tailed) IfferenceDifference Difference Lower Upper [Q20Al Equal variance 1.051 .306 -5.214 548 .000 -.3717 .07130 -.55603 -.1875 assume; Equal variances not assumed -5.225 546.476 .000 -.3717 .071 15 -.55565 -.I 878 IQ20B] Equal variancej .242 .623 -3.312 547 .001 -.2544 .0768] -.45296 -.0559 assume Equal variances not assumed -3.302 533.990 .001 -.2544 .07704 -.45358 -.0553 IQ20C1 Equafl variance .721 .396 ~5.926 557 .000 -.3736 .06304 -.53653 -.2106 assume Equal variances not assumed -6.000 549.643 .000 -.3736 .06226 -.53452 -.2126 02001 Equais variance 1.958 .000 -5.012 575 .000 -.2881 .05747 -.43661 -.1395 assume Equal variances not assumed -5.101 547.945 .000 -.2881 .05648 -.43406 -. l 421 IQ20E] Equafi variance 19.459 .000 -7.394 570 .000 -.4735 .06404 -.63905 -.3080 assume Egal variances not assumed -7.533 526.218 .000 -.4735 .06286 -.63604 -.31 10 Q20F1 Equza variance .113 .737 -3.156 571 .002 -.1960 .06210 -.35645 -.0355 assume Equal variances not assumed -3.170 570.630 .002 -.l960 .06182 -.35573 -.0362 0206] Equal variancej 5.410 .020 -2.582 566 .010 -.1548 .05994 -.30969 .0001 assume Equal variances not assumed -2.578 555.067 .010 -.1548 .06003 -.30994 .0004 209 Appendix G: Regression Analysis Output 210 Sample Population Results Descriptive Statistics Mean Std. Deviation N INTENT] 3.2345 .53775 496 KNOWALL 2.7883 .95428 496 GROUP .5020 .50050 496 LOC 3.6649 .58922 496 PERSONAL 3.7730 .60779 496 Q23 33.8931 18.96306 496 Q24 16.0464 13.93092 496 Q26 4.02 1 .324 496 Q27 3.48 1.054 496 Q28 6.24 2.049 496 Model Summary Model R R Square Adjusted R Std. Error of the Square Estlmate l .557 .310 .297 _45077 Predictors: (Constant), Q28, LOC, Q26, KNOWALL, GROUP, Q27, PERSONAL, Q24, Q23 Dependent Variable: INTENT] ANOVA M ode] Sum of d f Mean Squares Square 1 Regression 44.388 9 4.932 24.272 .000 Residual 98.753 486 .203 Total 143.14] 495 Predictors: (Constant), Q28, LOC, Q26, KNOWALL, GROUP, Q27, PERSONAL, Q24, Q23 Dependent Variable: INTENT] 211 Coefficients Unstandardized Standardized t Sig 95% Confidence Coefficients Coefficients ' Interval for B Std. Lower Upper Model 1 B Error Beta Bound Bound (Constant) 1.336 .188 7.116 .000 .967 1.705 KNOWALL -5.030E—03 .022 -.009 -.227 .821 -.049 .039 GROUP .148 .043 .137 3.426 .001 .063 .232 LOC .324 .039 .355 8.310 .000 .247 .400 PERSONAL .158 .039 .179 4.070 .000 .082 .234 Q23 -1.071E-03 .002 -.038 -.686 .493 -.004 .002 Q24 -2.322E—03 .002 -.060 -1.1 l l .267 -.006 .002 Q26 -1.065E-02 .022 -.026 -.492 .623 -.053 .032 Q27 4.816E-02 .023 .094 2.090 .037 .003 .093 Q28 6.289E-04 .01 1 .002 .059 .953 -.020 .022 Dependent Variable: INTENT] Residual Statistics (dependent variable: INTENTI) Unstandardized tandardized t Sig 95% Confidence Coefficients Coefficients ' Interval for B Std. Lower Upper Model 1 B Error Beta Bound Bound (Constant) 1.336 .188 7.116 .000 .967 1.705 KNOWALL -5.030E-03 .022 -.009 -.227 .821 -.049 .039 GROUP .148 .043 .137 3.426 .001 .063 .232 LOC .324 .039 .355 8.310 .000 .247 .400 PERSONAL .158 .039 .179 4.070 .000 .082 .234 Q23 -1.071E-03 .002 -.038 -.686 .493 -.004 .002 Q24 -2.322E-O3 .002 -.060 -1 .1 l 1 .267 -.006 .002 Q26 -1 .065E-02 .022 -.026 -.492 .623 -.053 .032 Q27 4.816E—02 .023 .094 2.090 .037 .003 .093 Q28 6.289E-04 .01 l .002 .059 .953 -.020 .022 212 Cluster 1: Individualists Descriptive Statistics Mean Std. Deviation N INTENT] 3.0596 .5712] 320 KNOWALL 2.6406 .98208 320 MORAL] 3.8153 .5221] 320 RESIST] 3.3010 .40889 320 LOC 3.5689 .63913 320 PERSONAL 3.6275 .59496 320 GROUP .0000 .00000 320 Q23 39.0280 18.95577 320 Q24 20.9189 15.79435 320 Q26 4.33 1.312 320 Q27 3.22 1.053 320 Q28 6.04 1.977 320 Model Summary R Adjusted Std. Error Change R Square R Of the Statistics Square Estimate R . F Slg. F Model (83:21:: Change dfl df2 Change 1 .563 .317 .295 .47964 .317 14.344 10 309 000 LOC, Q23, Q26 Dependent Variable: INTENT] Predictors: (Constant), Q28, Q24, PERSONAL, RESIST], KNOWALL, Q27, MORAL], ANOVA Model Sum 0f df Mean 1= Sig. Squares Square 1 Regression 32.999 10 3.300 14.344 .000 Residual 71.086 309 .230 Total 104.085 319 Predictors: (Constant), Q28, Q24, PERSONAL, RESIST], KNOWALL, Q27, MORAL] , LOC, Q23, Q26 Dependent Variable: INTENT] 213 Coefficients nstandardized Standardized t Sig Coefficients Coefficients ' Model B Std. Error Beta 1 (Constant) 1 .463 .338 4.325 .000 KNOWALL -3.91 1E-02 .029 -.067 -1.351 .178 MORAL] .352 .060 .322 5.828 .000 RESIST] -.172 .068 -.123 -2.523 .012 LOC .290 .052 .324 5.580 .000 PERSONAL 1.714E-02 .057 .018 .301 .764 Q23 -7.751E-04 .002 -.026 -.375 .708 Q24 9.441E-04 .003 .026 .363 .717 Q26 -5.537E-02 .030 -.127 -1.842 .066 Q27 2.297E-02 .030 .042 .761 .447 Q28 7.620E-04 .015 .003 .051 .959 Dependent Variable: INTENT] Residual Statistics Minimum Maximum Mean Std. Deviation N Predicted Value 2.0310 3.8620 3.0596 .32163 320 Std. Predicted Value -3.198 2.495 .000 1.000 320 Standard Error of Predicted Value .04432 .16521 .08639 .0211] 320 33:65“! Pred‘c‘ed 1.9828 3.8664 3.0586 .32267 320 Residual -1.6707 1.3591 .0000 .47206 320 Std. Residual -3.483 2.834 .000 .984 320 Stud. Residual -3.527 2.882 .001 1.003 320 Deleted Residual -1.7125 1.4058 .0010 .48994 320 it“? Deleted -3594 2.917 .000 1.007 320 esrdual Mahal. Distance 1.727 36.850 9.969 5.623 320 Cook's Distance .000 .056 .003 .006 320 86mm“ ”average .005 .116 .031 .018 320 alue Dependent Variable: INTENT] 214 Cluster 2: Egalitarians Descriptive Statistics Mean Std. Deviation N INTENT] 3.3622 .47909 279 KNOWALL 2.8566 .9450] 279 MORAL] 4.0207 .45336 279 RESIST] 2.2461 .4145 1 279 LOC 3.7307 .50986 279 PERSONAL 3.8806 .56444 279 Q23 30.8974 18.61404 279 Q24 13.4420 12.59847 279 Q26 3.80 1.306 279 Q27 3.68 1.018 279 Q28 6.49 1.911 279 Model Summary . Std. Ad usted R R JR Error Of Change Statistics Square Square the Estlmate R . Model Square Ch; 6 dfl dt2 Sign: Change g g l .555 .308 .282 .4059] .308 11.926 10 268 .000 Predictors: (Constant), Q28, LOC, Q26, RESIST], KNOWALL, MORAL1,Q27, PERSONAL, Q24, Q23 Dependent Variable: INTENT] ANOVA Model sum 0f df Mean F Sig. Squares Square 1 Regression 19.650 10 1.965 1 1.926 .000 Residual 44. 15 7 268 . 165 Total 63.808 278 Predictors: (Constant), Q28, LOC, Q26, RESIST], KNOWALL, MORAL1,Q27, PERSONAL, Q24, Q23 Dependent Variable: INTENT] 215 Coefficients Unstandardized Standardized t Sig Coefficients Coefficients ' Model B Std. Error Beta 1 (Constant) .794 .340 2.339 .020 KNOWALL -9.398E-03 .027 -.019 -.351 .726 MORAL] .259 .057 .245 4.571 .000 RESIST] -2.022E-02 .060 -.017 -.339 .735 LOC .256 .052 .272 4.883 .000 PERSONAL .104 .048 .122 2.139 .033 Q23 -3. 143E-06 .002 .000 -.002 .999 Q24 -2.174E-03 .003 -.057 -.825 .410 Q26 -2.471E-02 .026 -.067 -.943 .347 Q27 9.313E-02 .029 .198 3.262 .001 Q28 3.500E-03 .014 .014 .257 .797 Dependent Variable: INTENT] Residuals Statistics . . . Std. iMlnrmum Maxrrnum Mean Deviation N Predicted Value 2.1044 4.0493 3.3622 .265 86 279 Std. Predicted Value -4.731 2.585 .000 1.000 279 Standard Error of Predicted Value .03719 .22182 .0779] .02069 279 Adjuswd hemmed 1.7231 4.0861 3.3608 .27335 279 Value Residual -1.7356 1.1130 .0000 .39855 279 Std. Residual -4.276 2.742 .000 .982 279 Stud. Residual -4.402 2.806 .002 1.007 279 Deleted Residual -1.8392 1.2769 .0014 .41972 279 St“? Deleted -4.561 2.843 .001 1.013 279 Resrdual Mahal. Distance 1.337 82.025 9.964 7.163 279 Cook's Distance .000 .269 .005 .019 279 66”“ mange .005 .295 .036 .026 279 alue Dependent Variable: INTENT] 216 BIBLIOGRAPHY 217 BIBLIOGRAPHY Ajzen, I. (2002). Theory of Planned Behavior Diagram. Available: Http://www- unix.oit.umass.edu/~ajzen/tpb.diag.html [2002, April 27] Ajzen, l. (1991). The Theory of Planned Behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 50, 179-211. Ajzen, I. (1985). From Intentions to Actions: A Theory of Planned Behavior. in J. Kuhland & J. Beckman (Eds), Action-Control: From Cognitions to Behavior (pp. 11-39). Heidelberg: Springer. Ajzen, I. & M. Fishbein (1980). Understanding Attitudes and Predicting Social Behavior. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice Hall. Ajzen, I. & M. Fishbein (1977). Attitude - Behavior Relations: A Theoretical Analysis and Review of Empirical Research. Psychological Bulletin, 84(5), 888-918. Allen, J. B. & J .L. Ferrand (1999). Environmental Locus of Control, Sympathy, and Proenvironmental Behavior. Environment and Behavior, 31(3), 338-353. Arcury, T. A. & T.P. Johnson (1987). Public Environmental Knowledge: A Statewide Survey. Journal of Environmental Education, 18, 31-37. Arcury, T. A., Johnson, T. P. & SJ. Scollay (1986). Ecological Worldview and Environmental Knowledge: An Examination of The New Environmental Paradigm. Journal of Environmental Education, I 7, 35-40. Axelrod, L. J. (1994). Balancing Personal Needs with Environmental Preservation: Identifying the values that guide decisions in ecological dilemmas. Journal of Social Issues, 50(3), 85-104. Babbie, E. (1990). Survey Research Methods. (2nd Ed.). Belmount, CA: Wadsworth. Ball-Rokeach, S. J ., Rokeach, M. & J .W. Grube (1984). The Great American Value Test: Influencing Behavior and Belief Through Television. New York: Free Press. Bamberg, S., Kuhnel, S. M. & P. Schmidt (1999). The Impact of General Attitudes on Decisions. Rationality and Society, 11(1), 5-25. Bandura, A., Adams, N. E., Hardy, A. B. & G.N. Howells (1980). Tests of the Generality of Self-Efficacy Theory. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 4, 39-66. 218 Bardwell, L. (1991). Problem Framing: A Perspective on Environmental Problem Solving. Environmental Management, I 5, 603-6 12. Barrett, M. E. (2000, November). Structural BMPs.: The Solution to Water Quality Problems? Paper presented at the Water Quality And Quality Issues In Coastal Urban Areas, American Water Resources Association, Miami, FL. Bawden, R. (1995). Systemic Development: A Learning Approach to Change (pp. 67). Western Sydney: Centre For Systemic Development, University Of Western Hawkesbury. Bazerman, M. H., Messick, D. M., Tenbrunsel A.E. & K.A. Wade - Benzoni (1997). Environment, Ethics, and Behavior. San Francisco: New Lexington Press. Beckwith, J. A. & S.M. Ray] (2002). The Role of Knowledge and Responsibility in the Environmental Behavior of Mid-Westem University Students. Environmental Knowledge & Behavior, Article Under Review. Bengston, D. N. (1994). Changing Forest Values and Ecosystem Management. Society and Natural Resources, 7, 5 15-5 34. Biawas, A. B. (1973). Socio-Economic Consideration in Water Resources Planning. Water Resources Bulletin, 9, 746-754. Biawas, A. K. & RW. Durie (1971). Sociological aspects of Water Development. Water Resources Bulletin, 7, 1137-1143. Boerschig, S. & R. Deyoung (1993). Evaluation of Selected Recycling Curricula: Educating the Green Citizen. Journal of Environmental Education, 24(3), 17-22. Bosselrnann, Klaus (1997, October) Human Rights and the Environment: Redefining Fundamental Principles? Paper Presented at Environmental Justice: Global Ethics For The 21St Century, Melbourne, Australia Boster, F. (1999). Communications 901 : Path Analysis Class Text. Bright, A. D., & M.J. Manfredo (1997). The Influence of Balanced lnforrnation on Attitudes Towards Natural Resource Issues. Society and Natural Resources, 10, 469-483. Brown, G., & C. Harris (1992). The US. Forest Service: Towards the New Resource Management Paradigm? Society and Natural Resources, 5, 231-245. Burroughs, R. (1999). When Stakeholders Choose: Process, Knowledge, and Motivation in Water Quality Decisions. Society and Natural Resources, 12, 797-809. 219 Buss, D. M., Criak, K. H. & K.M. Dake (1986). Contemporary Worldviews and Perceptions of the Technological System. in V. T. Covello, J. Menkes, & J. Mumpower (Eds), Risk Evaluation and Management (pp. 93-130). New York: Plenum. Buss, D., Craik, K H. & KM. Dake (1985). Perceptions of Decision Procedures for Managing and Regulating Hazards. in F. Homburger (Ed.), Safety Evaluation and Regulation (pp. 199-208). New York: Kager. Carson, R. T. Louviere, J. J. & D.A. Anderson (1994). Experimental Analysis of Choice. Marketing Letters, 5, 351-368. Christianson, E. H. & T.A. Arcury (1992). Regional Diversity in Environmental Attitudes, Knowledge, and Policy: The Kentucky River Authority. Human Organization, 51(2), 99-108. Clean Water Act. (1972). 33 U.S.C. 1251-1387. Cohn, J. (1992). A Power Primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 155-159. Collins, H. M. & J. P. Pinch (1982). Frames of Meaning: The Social Construction of Extraordinary Science. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul. Conner, M. & C.J. Arrnitage (1998). Extending the Theory of Planned Behavior: A Review and Avenues for Further Research. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 28, 1429-1464. Converse, P. E. (1964). The Nature of Belief Systems in Mass Publics. in D. E. Apter (Ed), Ideology and Discontent (pp. 206-261). New York: The Free Press. Crosby, F., Bromley, S. & L. Saxe (1980). Recent Unobtrusive Studies of Black and White Discrimination and Prejudice: A Literature Review. Psychological Bulletin. 87, 546-563. Dake, K. (1992). Myths of Nature: Culture and the Social Construction of Risk. Journal of Social Issues, 48(4), 21-37. Dake, K. (1991). 'Orienting Dispositions in the Perceptions of Risk: An Analysis of Contemporary Worldviews and Cultural Biases. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 22, 61-82. Dillman, D. A. (2000). Mail and Internet Surveys. (2nd Ed.). New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons, Inc. Douglas, M. (1975). Implicit Meanings: Essays in Anthropology. london: Routledge & Kegan Paul. 220 Douglas, M. (1990). Risk as a Forensic Resource. Daedalus, 119(4), 1-16. Douglas, M. (1986). How Institutions Think. Syracuse, NY: Syracuse University Press. Douglas, M. (1985). Risk Acceptability According to the Social Sciences. New York: Russell Sage Foundation. Douglas, M. & A. Wildavsky (1982a). Risk and Culture: An Essay on the Selection of Technological and Environmental Dangers. Berkley: University Of California Press. Douglas, M. E. (1982b). Essays in the Sociology of Perceptions. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul. Dunlap, R. & R. Scarce (1991). The Polls & Poll Trends: Environmental Problems and Protection. Public Opinion Quarterly, 55, 713-734. Dunlap, R. E. & K Van Liere (1978). The New Environmental Paradigm. Journal of Environmental Education, 9, 10-19. Dunlap, R. E., Van Liere, K. D., Mertig, A. G. & R.E. Jones (2000). Measuring Endorsement of the New Ecological Paradigm: A Revised NEP Scale. Journal of Social Issues, 56(3), 425-442. Eckersley, R. (1992). Environmentalism and Political Theory: Towards an Ecocentric Approach. Albany: State University of New York. Eden, 8. E. (1993). Individual Environmental Responsibility And Its Role In Public Environmentalism. Environment And Planning, 25, 1743-1758. Edgell, M. C. R. & D.E. Nowell (1989). The New Environmental Paradigm Scale: Wildlife and Environmental Beliefs in British Columbia. Society and Natural Resources, 2, 285-296. Farley, J. U., Lehmann, D. R. & M.J. Ryan (1981). Generalizing from "Imperfect" Replication. Journal of Business, 54, 597-610. Feather, N. T. (1979). Value Correlates of Conservatism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 3 7(9), 1617-1630. Festinger, L. (1962). A Theory of Cognitive Dissonance, (2nd Ed.) California: Stanford University Press. Fishbein, M. & I. Ajzen (1975). Belief, Attitude, Intention and Behavior: An Introduction to Theory and Research. Reading, MA: Addision-Wesley. 221 Fiske, S. T. & S.E. Taylor (1990). Structures of Social Life: The Four Elementary F arms of Human Relations. New York, NY: The Free Press. Folger, R. (1996). Distributive and Procedural: Multi-Faceted Meaning and Interrelationships. Social Justice Research, 9, 371-394. Fortner, R W. & A.E. Lyon (1985). Effects of a Cousteau Television Special on Viewer Knowledge and Attitudes. Journal of Environmental Education, 16(3), 12-20. Fulton, D. C., Manfedo, M. J ., & J. Lipscomb (1996). Wildlife Value Orientation: A Conceptual and Measurement Approach. Human Dimensions of Wildlife, I , 24- 47. Furby, L. (1986). Psychology and Justice. in R. C. Cohen (Ed.), Justice: Views from the Social Sciences (pp. 153-203). New York, NY: Plenum Press. Furnham, A. F. (1988). Lay Theories: Everyday Understanding of Problems in the Social Sciences. Tarrytown, NY: Pergamon. Gallup, R (1989). Environment Regaining a F oothold on the National Agenda (Report 285). Princton, N.J.: Gallup Organization. Geller, S. E. (1995). Integrating Behaviorism and Humanism for Environmental Protection. Journal of Social Issues, 51(4), 179-195. Gregory, R., Keeney, R. & D. Von Winterfeldt (1992). Adapting the Environmental Impact Statement Process to Inform Decision Making. Journal of Policy Analysis and Management, I, 58-75. Guagnano, G. A. (1995). Locus of Control, Altruism and Agentic Disposition. Population and Environment: A Journal of Interdisciplinary Studies, 17(1), 63- 77. Hamilton, J. P. (1986). Environmental locus of Control as a Function of the Perceived Importance of Environmental Problems and Environmental Knowledge. Journal of Interpretation, 1 1(2), 15-31. Harris, D. H. (1977). The Human Dimensions of Water-Resources Planning. Human Factors, 19(3), 241-251. Herberlein, T. A. (1972). The Land Ethic Realized: Some Social Psychology Explanations for Changing Environmental Attitudes. Journal of Social Issues, 28(4), 79-87. 222 Hines, J. M., Hungerford, H. R. & A.N. Tomera (1986). Analysis Synthesis of Research on Responsible Environmental Behavior: A Meta-Analysis. Journal of Environmental Education, 18(2), 1-18. Homer, P. M. 8: L. R. Kahle (1988). Astructural Equation Test of the Value-Attitude- Behavior Hierarchy. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 5 4, 638-646. H00per, J. R., & J. M. Neilsen (1991). Recycling as Altruistic Behavior: Normative and Behavioral Strategies to Expand Participation in a Community Recycling Program. Environment and Behavior, 23(2), 195-220. Hrubes, D., Ajzen, I. & J. Daigle (2001). Predicting Hunting Intentions and Behavior: an Application of the Theory of Planned Behavior. Leisure Sciences, 23(4), 165-178. Hungerford, H. R. & T.L. Volk (1990). Changing learner Behavior Through Environmental Education. Journal of Environmental Education, 21(3), 8-21. Hwang, Y.-H., Kim, S.-I. & J.M. Jeng (2000). Examining the Causal Relationships among Selected Antecedents of Responsible Environmental Behavior. Journal of Environmental Education, 31(4), 19-25. Jackman, M. R. (1978). General and Applied Tolerance: Does Education Increase Commitment to Racial Integration? American Journal of Political Science, 22, 302-324. Jorgensen, B. S. & G. J. Syme (2000). Protest Responses and Willingness to Pay: Attitude Toward Paying for Storrnwater Pollution Abatement. Ecological Economics, 33(2), 251-265. Katz, P. A. (1976). The Acquisition of Racial Attitudes in Children. In P. A. Katz (Ed.), Toward the Elimination Of Racism (Pp. 125-154). New York: Pergamon. Kempton, W., Boster, B. & J. Harley (1995). Environmental Values in American Culture. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Kramer, R. M., McClintock, C. G. & D.M. Messick (1986). Social Values and Cooperative Response to a Simulated Resource Conservation Crisis. Journal of Personality, 54, 576-592. lane, R. (1973). Patterns of Political Belief. in J. Knutson (Ed.), Handbook of Political Psychology (pp. 83-116). San Francisco: Jossey—Bass. Lefcourt, H. M. (1982). Locus of Control - Current Trends in Theory and Research. (2nd Ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. 223 Lind, E. A., Kulik, C. T., Ambrose, M. & M. Park (1995). Procedural Justice and Arbitrated Dispute Resolution: The Role of Fairness Judgments in Individual and Corporate Decisions. Administrative Science Quarterly, 38(2), 224-252. Lind, E. A. (1992, March, 1992). The Fairness Heuristic: Rationality and "Relationality" in Procedural Evaluation. Paper presented at the fourth international conference of The Society For The Advancement Of Socio-Economics, Irvine, CA. Lind, A. E. & T.R. Tyler (1988). The Social Psychology of Procedural Justice. (Vol. 9). New York, NY: Plenum. Lovrich, N. P. J., Pierce, J. C., Tsurutani, T. & T. Abe (1986). Policy Relevant Information and Public Attitudes: IS Public Ignorance a Barrier to Nonpoint Pollution Management? Water Resources Bullein, 22, 229-236. Luzar, J. E. & KJ. Cosse' (1998). Willingness To Pay or Intention To Pay: The Attitude- Behavior Relationship in Contingent Valuation. Journal of Socio-Economics, 27(3), 427-444. Lynne, G. D. (1995). Conservation Technology Adoption Decisions and the Theory of Planned Behavior. Journal of Economic Psychology, 1 6, 581-598. MacCoun, R. J ., Lind, E. A., Hensler, D. R., Bryant, D. L. & P.A. Ebener (1988). Alternative Adjudication: An Evaluation of the New Jersey Automobile Arbitration Program. Santa Monica, CA: RAND Corporation: Institute of Civil Justice. Madden, T., Scholder Ellen, P. & I. Ajzen (1992). A Comparison of the Theory of Planned Behavior and the Theory of Reasoned Action. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 18(1), 3-9. Manfrado, M. J. & H.C. Zinn (1996). Population Change and its Implications for Wildlife Management in the New West: A Case Study of Colorado. Human Dimensions of Wildlife, 1, 62-74. Mars, J. (1982). Cheats at Work: An Anthropologv of Worlgolace Crime. London: Allen & Unwin. Maslow, A. H. E. (Ed.). (1959). New Knowledge in Human Values. New York: Harper. McConahay, J. M. (1986). Modern Racism, Ambivalence, and the Modern Racism Scale. in J. F. Dovidio & S. L. Gaertner (Eds), Prejudice, Discrimination, and Racism (pp. 91-124). Orlando, FL: Academic Press. 224 McFarlane, B.L. & P.C. Boxall (2000). Factors Influencing Forest Values and Attitudes of Two Stakeholder Groups: The Case of the Foothills Model Forest, Alberta, Canada, Society & Natural Resources 13: 649-661 McKenzie-Mohr, D., Nemiroff, L. S., Beers, L., & S. Desmarais (1995). Determinants of Responsible Environmental Behavior. Journal of Social Issues, 51(4), 139-156. Messick, D. M. (1993). Equity as a Decision Heuristic. in B. A. Mellers & J. Baron (Eds.), Psychological Perspectives on Justice: Theory and Applications (pp. 1 1- 31). New York: Cambridge University Press. Messick, D. M. & C.G. McClintock (1968). Motivational Bases of Choice in Experimental Games. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 4, 1-25. Meyer, Gary D. (1996). Meeting Public Expectations - Judicial Review of Environmental Impact Statements in the United States: Lessons for Reform in Western Australia? Murdock University Electronic Journal Of Law, 3(2) Michigan Information Center (MIC) (1999). County Estimates for Median Household Income for Michigan: 1998. State Of Michigan. Available: Http://www. state.mi.uS/dmb/mic/census/econ/98hhincome.htm [2002, March 15]. Miller, C. A. (2000). The Dynamics of Framing Environmental Values and Policy: Four Models of Societal Processes. Environmental Values, 9, 211-233. Montada, L. & E. Kals (2000). Political Implications of Psychological Research in Ecological Justice and Proenvironmental Behavior. International Journal of Psychology, 35(3), 168-176. Municipal Research & Services Center (MRSC) (2000). Best Management Practices (BA/H’s) For Storm and Surface Water. Available: http://www.mrsc.org/ environment/water/water-s/S W-BMP.htm [2001, August 13*] Nancarrow, B. E., Smith, L. M. & G.J. Syme (1996-7). The Ways People Think About Water. Journal of Environmental Systems, 25(1), 15-27. National Resource Conservation Service (NRCS) (1900). Sycamore Creek Watershed Water Quality Plan. Michigan State University Cooperative Extension Service, Agricultural Stabilization and Conservation Service, Ingham County, Michigan. Newhouse, N. (1990). Implications of Attitude and Behavior Research for Environmental Conservation. Journal of Environmental Education. 22(1), 26-32. O'Riordan, T. & S. Rayner (1991). Risk Management for Global Environmental Change. Global Environmental Change, 1(4), 91-108. 225 Parcels, G. S. (1984). Theoretical Models for Application in School Health Education Research. Journal of School Health, 54(6), 39-49. Peterson, R. S. (1994). The Role of Values in Predicting Fairness Judgments and Support of Affirmative Action. Journal of Social Issues, 50(4), 95-115. Phares, E. J. (1976). Locus of Control in Personality. Morristown, NJ: General Learning Press. Pierce, J. C. (1979). Conflict and Consensus in Water Politics. Western Political Quarterly, 32(3), 307-319. Pierce, J. C., Lovrich, N. P. J ., Tsurutani, T. & T. Abe (1989). Public Knowledge and Environmental Politics in Japan and the United States. Boulder, CO: Westview Press. Pierce, J. C., Steger, M. E., Steel, B. S. & N.P. Lovrich (1992). Citizens, Political Communication and Interest Groups: Environmental Organizations in Canada and the United States. Westport, CT: Praeger. Rasinski, K. A. (1987). What's Fair is Fair - Or is it? Value Differences Underlying Public Views About Social Justice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53(1), 201-211. Rayner, S. (19883). Muddling Through Metaphors to Maturity: A Commentary on Kasperson Et Al., The Social Amplification of Risk. Risk Analysis, 8, 201-204. Rayner, S. (1988b). The Rules That Keep US Equal: Complexity and the Costs of Egalitarian Organizations. In J. G. Flanagan & S. Rayner (Eds.), Rules, Decisions and Inequity in Egalitarian Societies (pp. 20-42). Aldershot, England: Avebury. Rayner, S. (1986). Management of Radiation Hazards in Hospitals: Plural Rationalities in a Single Institution. Social Studies of Science, 16, 573-591. Reading, R. P., Clark, T. W. & S.R. Kellert (1994). Attitudes and Knowledge of People Living in the Greater Yellowstone Ecosystem. Society and Natural Resources, 7, 349-365. Roesner, L. A., Bledsoe, B. P. & R.W. Brashear (2001). Are Best Management Practices Criteria Really Environmentally Friendly? Journal of Water Resources Planning and Management, May/June, 150 - 154. Rokeach, M. (1973). The Nature of Human Values. New York: Free Press. Rokeach, M. (1979). Understanding Human Values: Individual and Societal. New York: The Free Press. 226 Rotter, J. B. (1966). Generalized Expectancies for lntemal Versus External Control of Reinforcement. Psychological Monographs, 80(1). Rotter, J. B. (1975). Some Problems and Misconceptions Related to the Construct of Internal Versus External Control or Reinforcement. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 43(1), 56-67. Runes, D. (1983). Dictionary of Philosophy. New York, NY: Philosophical Library. Samdahl, D. M. & R. Robertson (1989). Social Determinants of Environmental Concern. Environment and Behavior, 21(1), 56-81. Satterfield, T. 8: R. Gregory (1998). Reconciling Environmental Values and Pragmatic Choices. Society and Natural Resources, 1 I , 629-647. Schumann, H., Stech, C. & L. Bobo (1985). Racial Attitudes in America: Trends and Interpretations. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Schwartz, S. H. (1992). Universals in the Content and Structure of Values: Theoretical Advances and Empirical Tests in 20 Countries. Advances on Experimental Social Psychology, 25, l - 65. Schwartz, M. & M. Thompson (1990). Divided We Stand: Redefining Politics, Technology and Social Choice. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press. Schwartz, S. H. & W. Bilsky (1987). Towards A Universal Psychology Structure of Human Values. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53(3), 550-562. Schwartz, S. H. (1977). Normative Influences on Altruism. in L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology (Vol. 10, pp. 221-279). New York: Academic Press. Schwartz, S. H. (1970). Moral Decision Making and Behavior. in J. Macauley & L. Berkowitz (Eds.), Altruism and Helping Behavior (Pp. 127-141). New York: Academic Press. Sears, D. O., & D.R. Kinder (1971). Racial Tensions and Voting Behavior in Los Angeles. in W. Z. Hirsch (Ed.), Los Angeles.: Viability and Prospects for Metropolitan Leadership (pp. 51-88). New York: Preager. Seligman, C., Syme, G. J. & R. Gilchrist (1994). The Role of Values and Ethical Principles in Judgments of Environmental Dilemmas. Journal of Social Issues, 50(3), 105-119. 227 Sheppard, B. H., Hartwick, J. & P.R. Warshaw (1988). The Theory of Reasoned Action: A Meta-Analysis of Past Research with Recommendations for Modifications and Future Research. Journal of Consumer Research, 15, 325-343. Shultz, P. W. & S. Oskamp (1996). Effort as a Moderator of the Attitude-Behavior Relationship: General Environmental Concern and Recycling. Social Psychology Quarterly, 59, 375-383. Sia, A., Hungerford, H. R. & A.N. Tommera (1985/86). Selected Predictors of Responsible Environmental Behavior: An Analysis. Journal of Environmental Education, 1 7(2), 31-40. Sidanius, J. & F. Pratto (1993). The Inevitability of Oppression and the Dynamics of Social Dominance. in P. Sniderman & P. Tetlock (Eds.), Prejudice, Politics, and The American Dilemma (pp. 173-211). Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press. Sivek, D. J. & H.R. Hungerford (1989). Predictors of Responsible Behavior in Members of Three Wisconsin Conservation Organizations. Journal of Environmental Education, 21(2), 35-40. Slovic, P. (1987). Perceptions of Risk. Science, 236, 280-285. Smith, D. G., Cragg, A. M. & G.F. Croker (1991). Water Quality Criteria for Bathing Waters Based on User Perception. Journal of Environmental Management, 33, 285-299. Smith, D. G. & R.J. Davies-Colley (1992). Perception of Water Clarity and Colour in Terms of Sustainable Recreational Use. Journal of Environmental Management, 36, 225-235. Smith-Sebasto, N. J. & R.W. Fortner (1994). The Environmental Action lntemal Control Index. Journal of Environmental Education, 25(4), 23-29. Smith-Sebasto, N. J. (1992). The Revised Perceived Environmental Control Measure: A Review and Analysis. Journal of Environmental Education, 23(2), 24-33. Sniderman, P. M., Brody, R. & J .H. Kuklinski (1991). The Principle-Policy Puzzle: The Paradox of American Racial Attitudes. in P. M. Sniderman, R. Brody, & P. E. Tetlock (Eds.), Reasoning and Choice (pp. 58-69). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Sniderman, P. M. & P.E. Tetlock (1986a). Interrelationship of Political Ideology and Public Opinion. in M. Hermann (Ed), Handbook of Political Psychology (2nd Ed., pp. 62-96). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. 228 Sniderman, P. M. & P.E. Tetlock (1986b). Symbiotic Racism: Problems of Motive Attribution. Journal of Social Issues, 42(2), 129-150. Steel, B., S, Soden, D. L. & R.L. Warner (1990). The Impact of Knowledge and Values on Perceptions of Environmental Risk to the Great Lakes. Society and Natural Resources, 3, 331-348. Steel, B. S. (1996). Thinking Globally and Acting locally? Environmental Attitudes, Behavior and Activism. Journal of Environmental Management, 47, 27 - 36. Steel, B. 8., List, P. & B. Shindler (1994). Conflicting Values About Federal Forests: A Comparison of National and Oregon Publics. Society and Natural Resources, 7, 137-153. Stern, P. C., Dietz, L., Kalof, L. & A. Guagnano (1995). Values, Beliefs, and Proenvironmental Action: Attitude Formulation Towards Emergent Attitude Objects. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 25 , 1611-1636. Stern, P. C. Dietz, T., & L. Kalof (1993). Value Orientations, Gender and Environmental Concern. Environment and Behavior, 25(3), 322 - 348. Sutton, S. (1998). Predicting and Explaining Intentions and Behavior: How Well Are We Doing? Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 28(15), 1377-1338. Syme, G. J. & M.D. Fenton (1993). Perceptions of Equity and Procedural Preferences for Water Allocation. Society and Natural Resources, 6, 347-360. Syme, G. J. & B.E. Nancarrow (1992). Predicting Public Involvement in Urban Water Management and Planning. Environment and Behavior, 24(6), 738-758. Syme, G. J. & B.E. Nancarrow (1996). Planning Attitudes, Lay Philosophies, and Water Allocation: A Preliminary Analysis and Research Agenda. Water Resources Research, 32(6), 1843-1850. Syme, G. J. & B.E. Nancarrow (1997a). Defining and Utilizing Lay Ethics and Equity in Land and Water Allocation. Paper presented at the at the Environmental Justice Conference, University Of Melbourne. Syme, G. J. & B.E. Nancarrow (1997b). The Determinants of Perceptions of Fairness in the Allocation of Water to Multiple Uses. Water Resources Research, 33(9), 2143-2152. Syme, G. J ., Nancarrow, B. E. & J .A. Mccreddin (1999). Defining the Components of Fairness in the Allocation of Water to Environmental and Human Uses. Journal of Environmental Management, 5 7, 5 1-70. 229 Syme, G. J ., & KD. Williams (19930). The Psychology of Drinking Water: An Exploratory Study. Water Resources Research, 29, 4003-4010. Tarrant, M. A., Bright, A. D. & H.K. Cordell (1997). Attitudes Towards Wildlife Species Protection: Assessing Moderating and Mediating Effects in the Value Attitude Relationship. Human Dimensions Of Wildlife, 2(2), 1-20. Tenbrunsel, A. E., Wade-Benzoni, K. A., Messick, D. M. & M.H. Bazerman (1997). The Dysfunctional Aspects of Environmental Standards. in M. H. Bazerman, D. M. Messick, A. E. Tenbrunsel, & K A. Wade-Benzoni (Eds.), Environment, Ethics and Behavior (pp. 105-121). San Francisco, CA: The New Lexington Press. Tetlock, P. E. (1986). A Value Pluralism Model of Ideological Reasoning. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 819-827. Thibaut, J. & L. Walker (1975). Procedural Justice: A Psychological Analysis. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Thompson, M. (1992). The Dynamics of Cultural Theory and Their Implications for the , Enterprise Culture. in A. Ross (Ed.), Understanding the Enterprise Culture . Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press. Thompson, M., Ellis, R. & A. Wildavsky (1990). Cultural Theory. Boulder, CO: Westview. Thompson, M. & P. James (1989). The Cultural Theory of Risk. in J. Brown (Ed), Environmental Threats: Perception, Analysis and Management (pp. 138-150). london: Belhaven. Thompson, M. (1988). Socially Viable Ideas of Nature. in E. Baark & U. Svedin (Eds.), Nature, Culture, Technology: Towards a New Conceptual Framework. London: Macmillan. Tyler, T. R. & E.A. Lind (1992). A Relational Model of Authority in Groups. in M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology (Vol. 25, Pp. 1 15-192). Orlando: Academic Press. Tyler, T. R. (1988). What is Procedural Justice? : Criteria Used by Citizens to Assess the Fairness of Legal Procedures. American Bar Foundation working Paper 8725 UNESCO. (1991). Our Common Future. Paris: UNESCO. US. Census 1990 (1991). United States Census Bureau. Washington, DC. US. Census 2000 (2001). United States Census Bureau. Washington, DC. 230 United States Environmental Protection Agency (USEPA) (1996, May 16, 200]). Eflluent Trading in Watersheds Policy Statement. Environmental Protection Agency [February 1996, April 12, 2002]. United States Environmental Protection Agency (USEPA) (1997). The Watershed Protection Approach. Environmental Protection Agency. Available: www.cpa.gov/owow/watershed/focuS/ref.htrnl [1997, January 27,1997]. United States Environmental Protection Agency (USEPA). (2000, 03/ 1 6/2001). Storm Water Phase II Final Rule. Office of Water. Available: www.cpagov/owm/sw/phaseZ [January 2000, April 2001] van den Putte, B. (1993). On the Theory of Reasoned Action. Unpublished Doctoral Dissertation, University Of Amsterdam, Amsterdam. Van Liere, K. D. & RE. Dunlap (1978). Moral Norms and Environmental Behavior: An Application of Schwartz's Norm Activation Model to Yard Burning. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 8(2), 174-188. Van Liere, K D. & R.E. Dunlap (1980). The Social Bases of Environmental Concern: A Review of Hypotheses, Explanations and Empirical Evidence. Public Opinion Quarterly, 44, 181-197. Vaske, J. J. & M.P. Donnelly (1999). A Value - Attitude - Behavior Model Predicting Wildland Preservation Voting Intentions. Society and Natural Resources, 12, 523- 537. Walster, E., Walster, W. G. & E. Berscheid (1978). Equity Theory and Research. Boston, MA: Allyn and Bacon Inc. Wenz, P. S. (1988). Environmental Justice. Albany: State University of New York Press. Widegren, O. (1998). The New Environmental Paradigm and Personal Norms. Environment And Behavior, 30, 75-100. Wildavsky, A. (1991). The Rise of Radical Egalitarianism. Washington, DC: American University Press. Woolrnington, E. & J .S. Burgess (1983). Hedonistic Water Use and Low Flow Runoff in Australia’s National Capital. Urban Ecology, Vol. 7, 215-227. Wynne, B. (1989). Frameworks for Rationality in Risk Management: Towards the Testing of Native Sociology. in J. Brown (Ed), Environmental Threats: Perceptions, Analysis, and Management (pp. 33-47). London: Belhaven. 231