LIBRARY Michigan State University PLACE IN RETURN BOX to remove this checkout from your record. TO AVOID FINES return on or before date due. MAY BE RECALLED with earlier due date if requested. DATE DUE DATE DUE DATE DUE “DEC 1 4 2005 6/01 cJCIRC/DateDuepGS-p. 15 MOTHER-INFANT ATTACHMENT: THE IMPACT OF MATERNAL REPRESENTATIONS DURING PREGNANCY, MATERNAL RISK FACTORS, AND SOCIAL SUPPORT By Alissa Christine Huth-Bocks A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Psychology 2002 ABSTRACT MOTHER-INFANT ATTACHMENT: THE IMPACT OF MATERNAL REPRESENTATIONS DURING PREGNANCY, MATERNAL RISK FACTORS, AND SOCIAL SUPPORT By Alissa Christine Huth-Bocks The mother-child relationship during infancy is considered to be one of the most important determinants of a child’s social and emotional development by clinical and developmental theorists. Because mother-infant attachment is so central to children’s well-being, it is critical to understand the mechanisms through which mother- infant attachment is formed. The theoretical framework for this study is based on both attachment theory and Daniel Stern’s (1995) recent formulation about the transition to motherhood. It examines the processes through which mother-infant attachment is formed, by investigating the effects of both individual and environmental factors, in a diverse and high-risk sample using a longitudinal research design. Two hundred and seven women were interviewed during their last trimester of pregnancy, two months alter birth, and one year after birth Participants were recruited throughout several counties in mid-Michigan. During pregnancy, mothers’ own experiences with caregivers during childhood were assessed, as were mothers’ thoughts and feelings about themselves as mothers and their soon-to-be infants. Mothers’ perceived level of social support and experiences with violence, depression, and poverty were assessed at multiple time points. Mother-infant attachment was assessed at 1 year postpartum using a well-known laboratory procedure. Structural equation modeling results indicated that individual and environmental variables measured during pregnancy and after birth had a significant impact on mother-infant attachment. The most important predictors were mothers’ own attachment experiences during childhood, representations of the infant-to-be during pregnancy, and psychosocial risk factors. The results from this study expand the current level of understanding regarding the development of different patterns of mother-infant attachment. The results also have important clinical implications for the treatment of mothers and young children at risk for psychopathology. Dedicated to the mothers and babies who participated in this study and taught me so much, to the little ones in my family who have given me so much joy, and to my husband who has loved and supported me throughout this work. iv ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS This dissertation would not Imve been possible without the care and assistance of many, many people. I would first like to express my deepest appreciation for my mentor, Alytia Levendosky, who has given generously in all ways during my six years of graduate school. In addition to specific guidance and support for this dissertation, she has done many things to help me grow professionally and personally—far too many to list here. I could not have asked for a better teacher, supervisor, and fi'iend, and I am very thankful to have spent so much time with her over these years. It has really been a wonderful experience for me. I would also like to thank the rest of my dissertation committee, who have all contributed in unique and important ways. I thank Anne Bogat for her support in my professional aspirations and her careful, meticulous way of carrying out our research (not to mention her wonderful cookingl), Alex von Eye for his patience throughout my statistical analyses and his funny sense of humor, and Hiram Fitzgerald for sharing his incredible knowledge about infant mental health and early childhood development and for challenging me to keep improving. I feel very fortunate to have learned from all of them. I am also grateful to Alytia, Anne, Alex, and Bill Davidson for their generous financial support of this dissertation, which was made possible through their many hours of grant writing. In addition, I want to thank my main partners in this research endeavor: Sally Theran, Robin Weatherill, and Jennifer Trotter. I could not have completed this without their daily emotional support and practical help. They are great fi'iends and I will always remember the fim we had together, even during highly stressful times. I will really miss them. Many, many thanks also go to the large number of dedicated, caring research assistants who have worked on this project and made this dissertation possrble. They spent endless hours recruiting subjects, interviewing families, and coding, scoring, and entering data. They too provided me with many much-needed laughs along the way. Finally, I would like to thank my family for their continued support, encouragement, confidence, and enthusiasm for my work. I have greatly appreciated their interest in what I do and their sharing in my achievements. TABLE OF CONTENTS List of Tables ......................................................................................... ix List of Figures ......................................................................................... x Introduction ............................................................................................ 1 Chapter 1: Theoretical Background ................................................................ 4 Development of Internal Working Models ...................................... 4 Categories of Attachment in Infancy ............................................. 8 Attachment Representations and Categories of Attachment in Adults. . . .. l 2 Chapter 2: Maternal Representations ............................................................ l6 Maternal Representations of Attachment and Mother-Infint Attachment ......................................................................... 16 Maternal Representations of the Infant ......................................... 27 Maternal Representations of Self-As-Mother ................................. 40 Chapter 3: Ecological Factors Related to Attachment ......................................... 49 Maternal Social Support and Mother-Infant Attachment .................... 49 Risk Factors and Mother-Infant Attachment .................................. 56 Chapter 4: Hypotheses of the Present Study .................................................... 61 Chapter 5: Method .................................................................................. 67 Participants ........................................................................ 67 Procedures ......................................................................... 68 Measures ........................................................................... 74 Chapter 6: Results .................................................................................. 89 Missing Data ....................................................................... 89 Data Reduction .................................................................... 90 Measurement Models ............................................................ 94 Full Structural and Measurement Model ....................................... 97 Exploratory Analyses: Concordance Between Attachment Groups ...... 101 Chapter 7: Discussion ............................................................................. 104 Representational Processes ..................................................... 104 Social Support and Mother-Infant Attachment .............................. 111 The Impact of Risk Factors ..................................................... 113 Exploratory Analyses ............................................................ 115 Summary of Results ............................................................. 120 Directions for Future Research ................................................ 124 Clinical Implications ............................................................. 126 Conclusions ....................................................................... 129 Appendices .......................................................................................... 130 Appendix A: F lyers for Recruitment .......................................... 131 Appendix B: Time 1 Consent Form ........................................... 133 Appendix C: Time 2 Verbal Assent Form .................................... 134 Appendix D: Tracking Letter and Information Sheet ....................... 135 Appendix E: Time 3 Consent Form ........................................... 137 Appendix F: Perceptions of Adult Attachment Questionnaire ............ 138 Appendix G: Working Model of the Child Interview ...................... 143 Appendix H: Maternal Eflicacy Questionnaire .............................. 150 Appendix I: The Strange Situation Laboratory Procedures ................ 152 Appendix J: Social Support Questionnaire ................................... 153 Appendix K: Postnatal Pregnancy Support Items ........................... 158 Appendix L: Demographic Questionnaire .................................... 159 Appendix M: Beck Depression Inventory .................................... 163 Appendix N: Severity of Violence Against Women Scales Time 1 ...... 166 Appendix 0: Severity of Violence Against Women Scales Time 3 ...... 168 Appendix P: Parent Behavior Checklist ...................................... 169 Appendix Q: Physical and Emotional Neglect Scale ........................ 170 Appendix R: Correlation Matrix of Original Model Variables ............ 171 Appendix S: PAAQ Measurement Model .................................... 173 Appendix T: WMCI Measurement Model .................................... 174 Appendix U: Time 1 Social Support Measurement Model ................ 175 Appendix V: Time 3 Social Support Measurement Model ................ 176 Appendix W: Risk Factors Measurement Model ........................... 177 Appendix X: Revised Risk Factors Measurement Model .................. 178 Appendix Y: Correlation Matrix of Final Model Variables ............... 179 Appendix Z: Standardized Residual Values for Observed and Latent Variable ........................................................... 181 Appendix AA: Lisrel 8.5 Syntax for Final Model .......................... 182 Appendix BB: Goodness of Fit Statistics .................................... 183 Appendix CC: Q-Plot of Standardized Residuals ........................... 184 References .......................................................................................... 186 LIST OF TABLES Table 1. Demographic Characteristics of Study Participants ................................. 69 Table 2. Descriptions of Original Attachment Classification by Subgroup ................ 82 Table 3. Descriptive Data for Measures in the Hypothesized Model .................... 91 -92 Table 4. Breakdown of Participants Experiencing Demographic Risks ..................... 93 Table 5. Breakdown of Participants Experiencing Depression .............................. 93 Table 6. Breakdown of Participants Experiencing Domestic Violence ..................... 93 Table 7. Breakdown of Participants with Child Abuse/Neglect Risk ....................... 93 Table 8. Breakdown of Participants Experiencing Depression and Domestic Violence at Time 1 .................................................................................... 96 Table 9. Concordance Between Three WMCI and SS Classifications .................... 102 Table 10. Concordance Between Two WMCI and SS Classifications .................... 103 LIST OF FIGURES Figure 1. Hypothesized Model of Prenatal and Postnatal Predictors of Mother-Infant Attachment at 12 Months ............................................................... 64 Figure 2. Best Fitting Model Depicting Prenatal and Postnatal Predictors of Mother- Infant Attachment at 12 Months ..................................................... 100 INTRODUCTION Beginning with the work of John Bowlby (1944, 1969) and Mary Ainsworth (Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters, & Wall, 1978), attachment theorists and researchers have been studying the nature and quality of mother-infant attachment, including the precursors and sequelae of different patterns of attachment. Empirical work has largely focused on the infant’siperspective of attachment with his/her mother by studying infiInt behaviors in stressful and non-stressful situations, from which attachment quality is inferred. More recently, theorists and researchers have begun to examine mother-infant attachment fi'om the parental perspective. In addition, there has been a trend to “move to the level of representation” rather than to approach the study of attachment solely at the behavioral level (Main, Kaplan, & Cassidy, 1985; Oppenheim & Waters, 1995). Both clinical reports (e.g., Frarherg, Adelson, & Shapiro, 1975) and recent empirical research have indicated that there is a significant relationship between mothers’ representations of their own childhood attachment and mothers’ attachment to their own infants (Benoit & Parker, 1994; Fonagy, Steele, Moran, Steele, & Higgitt, 1993; Fonagy, Steele, & Steele, 1991; Levine, Tuber, Slade, & Ward 1991; Slade & Cohen , 1996; Ward & Carlson, 1995). However, few empirical studies have been conducted to help clarify this continuity of attachment within families. Existing studies have suggested that maternal representations of the infant and of the self-as-mother might explain the relationship between mothers’ own attachment during childhood and the attachment formed with their infants (Ammaniti, 1991; Bretherton, Biringen, Ridgeway, Masline, & Sherman, 1989; Fava Vizziello, Antonioli, Cocci, & Invernizzi, 1993; Zeanah, Benoit, Hirshberg, Barton, & Regan, 1994). Unfortunately, most of these studies have been limited by small, primarily Caucasian and middle-class samples, as well as the use of different measures with no known psychometric properties. In addition to the impact of maternal representations on mother-infant attachment, other researchers have suggested that attachment is influenced by external factors such as the amount and type of maternal social support. Empirical studies have shown inconsistent results (Crockenberg, 1981; Zeanah et al, 1993); however, findings fiom different studies suggest that social support may have a more indirect effect on mother- infant attachment through factors such as maternal behaviors or maternal self-efficacy (Crockenberg, 1988). The current study examined possible mechanisms through which mothers’ own attachment experiences could impact the attachment relationship with their infants, as well as the impact of environmental factors on this relationship, within a large, diverse, and high-risk sample. This study used a longitudinal research design and previously established measures, and was conducted in conjunction with a larger study examining the impact of domestic violence during pregnancy on women and infants. Maternal representations of attachment, of the infant, and of the self-as-mother were assessed during pregnancy, while self-efficacy and mother-infant attachment were measured at 12 months after birth In addition, social support and several risk factors were measured both prenatally and postnatally. It was hypothesized that characteristics of both the mother and the mother’s environment would predict the quality of attachment formed with her infant. Because this study was largely based on attachment theory, an overview of this theory will be presented first in order to provide a fiamework for understanding the hypothesized model. Next, studies that have examined the relationship between mothers’ own attachment during childhood and mothers’ attachment to their infants will be reviewed. Although continuity of attachment has repeatedly been observed, possible mechanisms through which attachment is transmitted have generally been missing in the literature. Two possible mechanisms appear to be mothers’ representations of their infants and representations of themselves as mothers, and the existing literature in these areas will be reviewed and critiqued. Since the present study also examined the relationship between social support and attachment, the few studies that have examined this relationship will be reviewed and their mixed findings will be discussed. Finally, because this study examined factors related to mother-infant attachment within a high- risk sample, literature that describes the relationship between a number of potential risk factors and mother-infant attachment will be explored. CHAPTER 1: THEORETICAL BACKGROUND Attachment Theory and the Mother-Infant Relationship The mother-infant relationship has been of interest to psychoanalytic theorists since its inception. Sigmund Freud initially recognized the importance of the mother as an “Object” that gratifies the infant’s instinctual drives and fiequently noted the profound effect that early relationships had on later fimctioning. It was not until the development of object relations theory, by key figures such as Donald Winnicott, William Fairbairn, and John Bowlby, that this relationship became a more central focus of inquiry. Unlike previous analytic theorists, object relations theorists maintained that the infant is primarily “driven” to establish relatedness and interactions with others as an end in itself. They purported that infants come “wired” or prepared for immediate human interactions, which form the foundation for the infant’s developing sense of selfand future relationships. In addition, clinical investigators such as Rene Spitz helped clarify the infant’s role as an active participant in dyadic relationships, which furthered our understanding of self-object differentiation. Drawing fi'om evolutionary biology, cognitive psychology, and ethology, in addition to object relations theory, John Bowlby (1969, 1988) developed attachment theory to further explain the mother-infant relationship. Bowlby stated that the infant comes into the world with an innate predisposition toward social interaction as a means of survival and adaptation. Thus, the infant is driven to seek proximity with the primary caregiver, particularly when threat or danger is perceived, because s/he is unable to protect him/herself. According to Bowlby (1969), infimts are born with a number of attachment behaviors that are used to regulate the attachment relationship with the mother, including sucking, smiling, crying, and clinging, the rmin purpose of which are to attain or maintain proximity to a caregiver who is conceived as better able to cope with the world. Bowlby proposed that attachment behaviors are part of a behavioral system that help maintain a sense of emotional homeostasis for the infant. Within this system, infant behaviors are activated when a distance threshold between the infant and caregiver is exceeded or another threat is perceived (Carlson & Sroufe, 1995). In a well-firnctioning system, parents contribute to this homeostasis by showing complementary attachment behaviors, which are of the caregiving type. While the infant behaves in ways that seek out help and protection, mothers behave in ways that protect and secure the infant. When proximity is achieved, infant attachment behaviors are deactivated. As will be described further, parents and infants can also develop more maladaptive attachment patterns during which infant security is compromised. Mother-infant attachment appears to develop within the first few months of life (Bowlby, 1988), possibly because it is during this time that infant regulation (or dysregulation) has long-term effects on neural system development (Carlson & Sroufe, 1995). During the first 3 months of life, a synchrony related to physiological needs sets the tone for later psychological regulation. Between 3 and 6 months, the caregiver helps the infant maintain organized behaviors and afiect regulation in the face of stress and arousal by making stress tolerable. This enables the infant to feel secure even when faced with negative feelings and to gain confidence that s/he can influence what lappens to him/her. Around the ages of6 to 9 months, the infant assumes a more active role in self- regulation and self-initiated activities due to increased development of motor and cognitive abilities. With the onset of locomotion, additional attachment behaviors are added to the infant’s repertoire such as purposeful approach to the mother, following the mother, and use of the mother as a secure base from which to explore (Ainsworth, 1967), all of which continue to serve the function of maintaining proximity to the mother when needed. Development of Internal Workgg' Models By the end of the first year, Bowlby (1969, 1980) proposed that infants begin to develop “internal working models,” which are mental representations of self and others based on early experiences with caregivers (a more elaborated version of what other psychoanalysts called “internal objects” or “internal world”). Internal working models are formed fi‘om the history of the infant’s actions, infant-parent interactions, and the caregiver’s responses to the infant, rather than fiom an objective picture of the parent (Main et al., 1985), and working models of self and other are believed to be separate, but complementary. Although internal working models are thought to consist of generalized representations, Bowlby preferred the term “internal working model” because it emphasizes the dynamic and firnctional aspects of representations (Bretherton, Ridgeway, & Cassidy, 1990). Bowlby stated that the function of internal working models is to help the infant interpret and anticipate others’ behaviors and to guide his/her own behaviors in relationships, which is adaptive to the individual. For example, the infant can operate internally by making a plan am! selecting behaviors to achieve a goal before action is needed (Marvin & Britner, 1999). Although early internal working models are not necessarily fixed, they tend to show stability and resistance to change because a) they are partly unconscious (Bowlby, 1973, 1980; Bretherton, 1990; Cassidy, 1990), b) they bias what individuals expect and perceive, leading to some distortion of incoming information (Bretherton, 1990; Bretherton & Munholland, 1999), and c) reconstruction requires sigm'flcantly different emotional experiences that do not fit with the current working model (Bretherton et al., 1990; Ricks, 1985). Bretherton and colleagues (1990, 1999) recently extended Bowlby’s ideas about working models by considering advances in the cognitive sciences (e.g., memory research). She proposed that internal working models are best conceptualized as hierarchically organized schemas (or representations), with lower levels consisting of specific, interactional schemas and higher levels becoming more and more general as they subsume lower-level schemas. For example, beginning at the lowest level and proceeding to the highest level, one part of a working model might be: “When I fall down and cry, my mother picks me up,” “When I am hurt, my mother comforts me,” “My mother is there when I need her,” and finally “My mother is a loving person.” Bretherton ( 1990) also stated that internal working models of self and others are probably several inter-linked hierarchies of representations, rather than a single hierarchy. In addition, she noted that there are individual difierences in the degree of organization and consistency within and across levels, which have an effect on a person’s interpretation of events and subsequent behaviors. For example, individuals who have representations that are not linked properly or are dissociated from one another will misinterpret signals from the environment and will be more likely to show contradictory behaviors. Further, incoming information may not be generalized throughout the hierarchies, which may lead to overly rigid working models. Individuals with such distorted, “ill-organized” hierarchies are more likely to be insecurely attached than individuals with more well-organized and consistent representations. Internal working models of the self are similar to other working models, i.e., of the attachment figure, as they are mental constructions based on actual experiences that guide appraisals, feelings, and behaviors (Cassidy, 1990). Working models of the self develop out of events and interactions and are believed to be generalized representations of the self. Further, working models of the self contain both cognitive and afl‘ective components, that is, ideas about self-image (“I am a hard worker”) and self-esteem (“I am lovable”) (Bowlby, 1979; Cassidy, 1990). Based on internal working models of the self and of others, infants interact in certain ways with their mothers during times of perceived threat. Under stressful conditions, infants either have their attachment needs met or they find other ways to cope with their unmet needs, i.e., by exaggerating signals or suppressing their needs (Barnett & Vondra, 1999). Therefore, based on early experiences with caregivers, individual differences in attachment develop. Differences in attachment are believed to be finite and can be classified into several central patterns, which are “organized” in the sense that they reflect a consistent, coherent strategy for behaving in relation to the caregiver (Ainsworth et al., 1978; Main et al., 1985). These categories of attachment will be summarized next. Categories of Attachment in Infancy Mary Ainsworth, a student of John Bowlby’s, made substantial contributions to attachment theory through her early observations of mother-infant dyads (Ainsworth, 1967). As a result of her work, she created a procedure to empirically study mother- infant attachment called the Strange Situation (Ainsworth et al., 1978). This brief, mildly stressful, laboratory procedure consists of a series of interactions between the infant, the mother, and a stranger, during which the infant is separated and reunited with the mother twice. The infant’s behaviors during reunion with the mother are believed to be indicative of the infant’s internal working model of the relationship (Main et al., 1985), and therefore, quality of attachment is inferred fiom Observed behaviors. A Secure Attachment (Type B) is characterized by the infant’s confidence that a caregiver will be available and responsive when in need. As attachment theory would suggest, research has shown that a secure attachment is associated with a history of consistent, sensitive, responsive care, which presumably gives rise to an internal working model of others, and the world in general, as dependable, comforting, and protective. Because these infants can rely on their caregivers, they are able to safely explore the environment and are likely to develop a sense of mastery and control. At the same time, securely attached children develop an internal working model of the self as valuable, worthy of love, and self-reliant. Based on these internal working models, securely attached children show unique behaviors in the Strange Situation. They are able to use the caregiver as a secure base from which to explore the room. Upon separation, these infants may be distressed and may be comforted by a stranger, but will show a clear preference for the mother and will seek contact with her upon reunion. In addition, these children will be readily comforted by the mother and often return to exploration and play (see Carlson & Sroufe, 1995; Weinfield, Sroufe, Egeland, & Carlson, 1999 for reviews). Research has shown that approximately 60-75% of low-risk infants display a secure pattern of attachment (van Ijzendoom & Kroonenberg, 1988). The internal working model of an infant Showing an Insecure-A voidant Attachment (Type A) is characterized by a lack of confidence in the caregiver and expectations of rejection. The caregiving history of these children include overt rejection, intrusive and controlling behaviors, as well as disengagement by the caregiver. Internal working models of the self likely include a view of the self as unworthy of care, but also possibly an extreme view of self-reliance in the context of a relationship in which the infant receives little care and comfort. Infants displaying this pattern of attachment in the Strange Situation show little distress at separation, no preference for the mother, and often actively avoid the mother upon reunion by ignoring her, looking or turning away, and resisting her initiation of contact. In fact, these infants show almost no afi‘ective responses at all and may appear indifferent. Avoidant behavior is believed to reflect a defensive strategy (namely repression) against emotioml pain associated with prior parental rejection, as well as a strategy to prevent the direct expression of anger that might put the child at risk for alienating the attachment figure (Bowlby, 1980; Cassidy & Kobak, 1988). Evidence in support for this notion comes from studies showing a significantly higher level of reactivity in avoidant infants as measured by cortisol levels and cardiac firnctioning (e.g., Gunner, Mangelsdorf, Larson, & Hertsgaard, 1989). Approximately 15-25% of infants in the general population have an avoidant attachment. Insecure-Ambivalent Attachment (Type C), seen in about 10-15% of low-risk samples, occurs when the infant is uncertain whether a parent will be available and responsive. Insecure-ambivalent infants generally lack confidence in the world because caregivers have been unresponsive and inconsistently available for meeting their children’s attachment needs. As a result of such experiences, parallel internal working 10 models of selfas unworthy and/or incompetent develop. During the Strange Situation, these children seem preoccupied with their caregivers and Show a paucity of exploration and play. Upon separation, these infants show a high level of overt distress and are especially wary of strangers. Upon reunion, ambivalent infants are not able to be soothed and display anger and ambivalence toward the caregiver by seeking and resisting contact at the same time. Unlike avoidant infants who may suppress attachment behaviors and needs, ambivalent infants are believed to exaggerate attachment needs and signals in an effort to get a response fiom the caregiver and to ensure the mother’s availability (see Cassidy & Berlin, 1994 for a review). Afier a number of studies discovered that about 10-15% of infants could not be reliably classified into the A, B, or C patterns of attachment, a fourth pattern was identified by Main and Solomon (1990), which they termed Disorganized-Disoriented Attachment (Type D). Disorganized infants are believed to experience an unusually high degree of fear within the attachment relationship, and research has shown that these caregivers are often abusive, neglectfirl, and/or traumtized by their own experiences of loss or violence (Main & Hesse, 1990; Main & Solomon, 1990; Lyons Ruth, Bronfrnan, & Parsons, 1999). Unlike the three traditional groups, infants with this attachment pattern appear to lack a coherent strategy for coping with stress and interacting with their caregivers. Thus, their behaviors in the Strange Situation are idiosyncratic and may include apprehensive or depressed behavior, alterations between approach and avoidance toward the caregiver, strange behaviors such as fi'eezing or stereotypies, and disorientation reflected by wandering, confusion, and rapid changes of affect (Main & Solomon, 1990). 11 Attachment Representations and Categories of Attachment in Adults Although attachment has historically been studied at the behavioral level with mother-infant dyads, as in the Strange Situation, recent advances have made it possible to study attachment at the “level of representation” with older individuals (Main et al., 1985). Main et al. proposed that internal working models formed during infancy function as a “template” through which experiences are filtered. Thus, internal working models of self and others tend to be fairly consistent throughout childhood and into adulthood. On the basis of these aspects of attachment theory, Main and colleagues (George, Kaplan, & Main, 1985) developed the Adult Attachment Interview (AAI) to assess adults’ representations of attachment or internal working models. These researchers suggested that internal working models influence feelings and behaviors, as well as attention, memory, and thinking processes as they relate to attachment. Therefore, individual difl'erences in attachment representations will manifest themselves in the way one speaks about attachment experiences and the way in which one constructs narratives about early experiences with caregivers. Although most research in this area has used the AAI to measure adults’ representations of attachment, it should be noted that several questionnaires have also been developed to measure adults’ attachment experiences during childhood (e.g., Epstein, 1983; Lichtenstein & Cassidy, 1991). Based on responses to the AAI, researchers are able to infer a person’s overall state of mind with respect to his/her own attachment experiences. Emphasis is placed on the way in which a person recalls memories and experiences fiom childhood (the narrative process), rather than on what they recall (the content). Therefore, special attention is paid to the degree of coherence and integration of a person’s narrative. The 12 AAI coding system, developed by Main and Goldwyn (1984a, 1998), allows people to be classified into one of four attachment categories. Adults classified as Autonomous tend to produce consistent and coherent narratives and are able to integrate positive and negative aspects of feelings and experiences. In addition, autonomous adults tend to value attachment relationships and believe that they influence current behavior. Dismissing adults are likely to minimize attachment-related experiences and tend to give short, inconsistent responses. Although these adults often report highly positive experiences, they are unable to provide examples to support their favorable representations. It is not uncommon for dismissing adults to have trouble remembering aspects of their childhood, and some researchers have suggested that the dismissing style and tendency to idealize may reflect a defensive strategy against painful memories (Cassidy & Kobak, 1988). Adults classified as Preoccupied also display inconsistent and/or incoherent interviews; however, they seem preoccupied with past attachment experiences. They appear to be greatly influenced by earlier experiences in a rather negative manner (i.e., they display much anger or fear). Adults can also be classified as Unresolved if they fail to maintain an organized narrative or show striking lapses in reasoning or strange use of speech, particularly when discussing traumatic events (George et al., 1985; Hesse, 1999). Finally, these four groups are thought to parallel the four types of attachment observed in infants during the Strange Situation (secure, insecure-avoidant, insecure-ambivalent, and disorganized, respectively). In sum, attachment theory suggests that infants develop internal working models, or representations of self and others, based on early interactions with caregivers. 13 Individual differences in working models exist, and subsequently, infants behave differently toward their mothers and display different patterns of attachment to their mothers, which may be more readily seen during times of stress. Attachment theory suggests that internal working models are relatively stable through childhood and into adulthood, and thus, adults maintain representations of attachment based on early experiences with caregivers. Some studies have firrther suggested that mothers “transmit” their own attachment patterns to their infants, demonstrating a stability of attachment across generations. Based largely on attachment theory, Daniel Stern (1985, 1995) has recently developed a unique framework for understanding the process through which mother- infant attachment is formed and possibly transmitted intergenerationally. Stern (1995) proposes that mothers develop a unique, psychic organization during pregnancy, which he terms “the motherhood constellation,” composed of a new set of fantasies, fears, action tendencies, and wishes never before experienced and directly due to becoming a mother. Stern believes that this psychological change begins during pregnancy, particularly after the first trimester of pregnancy when the fetus begins to move, and the reality of the baby becomes more apparent. In addition to the mother’s representations of self and other, which become activated and reworked during pregnancy, Stern (1995) suggests that the infant’s developing representations are an important component of the mother-infant relationship. Like Bowlby and other attachment theorists, Stern emphasizes that the infant’s representations are constructed from interactive experiences with the caregiver. However, he notes that rather than viewing representations as internalized events or 14 Objects, representations are built fiom within, “from the experience of being with another.” Similar to Bretherton’s notion of multiple, hierarchically organized internal working models, Stern (1995) suggests that repeated interpersonal experiences (or “emergent moments”) give rise to a network of schemas, which form an overall schema- of-being-with. Furthermore, he notes that schemas-of-being-with that are tied together by a common theme may be considered a ‘Tepresentation-of-being-with.” Thus, Stern suggests that schemas-of-being-with are hierarchically ordered fiom actual interactions to more generalized internal working models, the latter of which ultimately guide behavior. Consistent with attachment theory, Stern (1995) indicates that representations-of- being-with and internal working models are relatively stable throughout childhood and adulthood. Like other researchers, Stern also suggests that mothers’ representations of attachment may be transmitted or passed on to their own infants, resulting in a continuity of attachment across generations. Empirical studies that have investigated the relationship between mothers’ own representations of attachment and their attachment with their infants will be reviewed next, followed by a review of the literature examining the possible mdiating role of other types of maternal representations, such as representations of the infant and of the self. 15 CHAPTER 2: MATERNAL REPRESENTATIONS Maternal Representations of Attachment and Mother-Infant Attachment Psychoanalytic theory and clinical papers have long suggested that there is an intergenerational pattern of parent-child relationships, that is, that history repeats itself (e.g., Freud, 1940; Bowlby, 1969, 1980; Fraiberg et al., 1975). Although Fraiberg et al. stated “history is not [necessarily] destiny,” they convincingly described clinical cases that demonstrated how the conflicted past of the parents interfered with the relationship with their children. That in fact, a mother’s own past experiences as a child were being repeated in striking detail with her own child. Early writings such as these, however, had little empirical work to support their assertions. No systematic investigations had been conducted to determine the factors that led to the repetition of the past or (in fewer cases) that blocked the repetition. With the development of the AAI, researchers have been able to empirically examine the intergenerational patterns of relationships and the impact of mothers’ own representations of attachment on mother-infant attachment. In several early studies, mothers’ responses on the AAI and AAI attachment classifications were compared to infants’ behaviors and attachment security observed during the Strange Situation (Grossman, Fremmer-Bombik, Rudolph, & Grossman, 1988; Main & Goldwyn, 1984b; Main et al., 1985). The Strange Situation was administered to middle-class, mother- infitnt dyads when infants were 12 months old (N ranged fiom 40 to 65 in different studies), and the AAI was later administered to the same mothers when their children were 5 to 6 years old. Main et a1. (1985) found that mothers’ own attachment security was significantly related to their infants’ attachment security. Further, dismissive 16 mothers were more likely to be parents of avoidant infants, while preoccupied mothers were more likely to have ambivalent infants. Main and Goldwyn (1984b) also noted a striking similarity between a mother’s own experiences of rejection, her rejection of her infant, and the infant’s subsequent avoidant behaviors, suggesting that specific behaviors may also be repeated. Grossman et a1. (1988), using their own coding system for the AAI made up of four scales that were believed to reflect a parent’s attachment representations, reported that mothers with a representation of their parents as supportive and mothers who were able to focus on attachment experiences in a non-defensive way (even if these experiences were negative) were significantly more likely to have secure infants, while mothers who presented defensive representations (i.e., unable to discuss experiences or to recall memories) were more likely to have insecurely attached infants. Thus, overall, these studies indicated that mother-infant attachment patterns tended to replicate mothers’ own attachment experiences and representations. The generalizability of results fi'om these studies are limited by the predominately upper-middle class, educated, Caucasian sample. In addition, because the AAI was administered several years after the Strange Situation, it is more difficult to conclude that maternal representations led to infant attachment style. That is, it is possible that having children for a number of years somehow changed a mother’s own representations of her relationship with a caregiver. However, attachment theory suggests that intemal working models are fairly stable over time, and therefore, mothers’ representations of attachment were presumably simihr when infants were 12 months old. 17 Mm RgnesengtionsAQd Infant Attach_n_r_ent Measured Concurrenm Several other studies have examined the continuity of attachment by measuring both mothers’ representations of attachment and infant attachment concurrently (Pederson, Gleason, Moran, & Bento, 1998; Zeanah et al., 1993). Researchers in both studies administered the AA] and Strange Situation to a sample of 60 middle-class, mostly married, Caucasian mothers with 1 year-old infants. Using the original AAI coding system, both studies reported a significant degree of concordance between mothers’ own attachment representations and their infants’ attachment categories (73% agreement with a kappa = .56 and 75% agreement with a kappa = .62, respectively). By collapsing insecure types, Pederson et al. found that the concordance rate between autonomous-secure and nonautonomous-insecure groups increased to 80% (kappa = .60). However, when examining specific types of attachment, the researchers found that although there was a significant concordance between dismissive mothers and avoidant infants (69% and 94%, respectively) and between autonomous mothers and secure infants (81% and 87%, respectively), there was no significant relationship between preoccupied mothers and ambivalent infants. These studies suggest that the degree of concordance between mothers’ and infants’ patterns of attachment may be unique to each type of attachment classification, with more stability for the dismissive-avoidant and autonomous-secure groups and less stability for the preoccupied-ambivalent groups, at least among a Caucasian, middle-class sample. Pederson et a1. (1998) attempted to examine the process through which attachment was transmitted by assessing maternal sensitivity as a possible mediator. They discovered that although matemal sensitivity was significantly related to mothers’ 18 own security of attachment and to infant security of attachment, sensitivity accounted for less than 25% of the association between mother and infant attachment. Therefore, it seems important to continue to assess possrble mechanisms that might help explain the intergenerational continuity of attachment patterns, as well as to examine these relationships in larger, more diverse samples. Finally, in discussing the social transmission of parenting and attachment, Ricks (1985) examined the relationship between mothers’ recollections of relationships with parents and current mother-infant attachment in a sample of 24 middle-class mother- infant dyads. Ricks used the Mother-Father-Peer Scale (Epstein, 1983), which is a questionnaire assessing the degree of parental acceptance/rejection, independence/overprotection, and idealization of parents, rather than the AAI, as the measure of matemal attachment. As expected, mothers of securely attached infants reported more positive recollections of childhood relationships including more acceptance, encouragement, and less rejection than mothers of insecurely attached infants. However, there were no difi‘erences between mothers on amount of idealization. Unfortunately, concordance rates between attachment categories were unable to be examined, as seen in other studies, due to the type of measure used in this study. Existing studies that have assessed maternal representations of attachment concurrently or years after the assessment of mother-infant attachment have provided fairly strong empirical support for the continuity of attachment across generations. Furthermore, a few studies have shown a significant concurrent relationship between maternal representations based on the AA] and preschooler attachment security, suggesting that maternal representations may continue to influence young children’s l9 developing representations of attachment after infancy (Bus & van Ijzendoom, 1992; Eiden, Teti, & Corns, 1995; Posada, Waters, Crowell, & Lay, 1995). However, a growing number of research studies have utilized a prospective research design, by examining maternal representations of attachment before the infant is even born and then assessing mother-infant attachment at 1 year postpartum. In contrast to cross-sectional studies, a prospective design beginning in pregnancy increases confidence in the conclusion that maternal representations contribute to the development of mother-infant attachment rather than sinme being related correlationally. Maternal Representations and Infant Attachment Measured Prom One such study administered the AAI to a sample of 96 pregnant, Caucasian women fiom middle to upper-middle class backgrounds and then administered the Strange Situation when infants were 1 year old (Benoit & Parker, 1994). Using the four- classification system, results revealed a significant degree of concordance between mothers’ representations of attachment during pregnancy and mother-infant attachment at 1 year (68% match, kappa = .46). Using three groups only (by reclassifying the unresolved/disorganized type into one of the other insecure groups), the authors reported an 81% concordance rate (kappa = .55). However, consistency across generations secured to difler for secure and insecure groups; 83% of autonomous mothers had secure infants, while only 22% of dismissing mothers had avoidant infants, 57% of preoccupied mothers had ambivalent infants, and 58% of unresolved mothers had disorganized infants. Thus, the authors concluded that the highly significant rate of stability across generations was primarily due to the autonomous-secure groups while insecure groups seemed to be significantly less predictable over time. 20 Interestingly, Benoit and Parker (1994) also administered the AAI postnatally and found that AAI responses were significantly stable over time (77% match rate using the four groups and 90% using three groups), suggesting that mothers’ states of mind about attachment experiences are highly stable. The authors also reported that concordance rates were slightly higher for the AAI measured concurrently with the Strange Situation (74% using four groups and 82% using three groups), compared to pregnancy AAIs, although this was again primarily due to the autonomous-secure groups. Finally, this study was the first to examine patterns of attachment across three generations by also administering the AAI to the pregnant women’s own mothers. Results fi'om these analyses revealed an overall concordance rate of 65% using the three classification groups across all three generations. Although the findings from this study are impressive in a number of ways (e.g., prospective design, sample size, and data fi'om three generations), the results must be interpreted with caution due to the skewed distribution towards attachment security (76% of infants were secure using three groups) and the limited generalizability fiom a non-representative sample. Fonagy and colleagues have also extensively studied the intergenerational continuity of attachment beginning with maternal representations during pregnancy (Fonagy et al., 1991; Fonagy, Steele, Steele, Moran, &. Higgitt, 1991; Fonagy et al., 1993; Steele, Steele, & Fonagy, 1996). In an early study, they administered the AAI during the last trimester of pregnancy to a fairly large sample (N = 100) of Caucasian, well educated and mostly married women and then administered the Strange Situation when infants were 1 year old (Fonagy et al., 1991; Fonagy et al., 1991). They reported an overall match between the three original classifications of 66% (kappa = .38) and a 75% match 21 rate (kappa = .48) when two-way comparisons were made (secure-insecure). When examining concordance between specific groups, they found that 75% of autonomous mothers had secure infants, 68% of dismissive mothers had avoidant infants, but only 20% of preoccupied mothers had ambivalent infants. These findings are similar to those in other studies (Pederson et al., 1998; Zeanah et al., 1993), which suggest less stability for the preoccupied/ambivalent groups, and more generally, for insecure types of attachment. In an effort to understand the discontinuity between preoccupied mothers and ambivalent infants, Fonagy et al. (1991) discovered that a number of mothers who later had insecure-ambivalent infants had a unique interview style during pregnancy. They found that many of these mothers initially presented an overall impression of security in their representations of attachment (and therefore were rated as being autonomous), but upon closer examination, tended to have a somewhat exaggerated sense of security in combination with difficulties adjusting to their new maternal role (i.e., less confidence in themselves). On the other hand, one-third of preoccupied mothers went on to have a secure attachment pattern with their infants, suggesting tlmt positive changes in attachment representations are possible as well, perhaps even made more likely by becoming a parent. These are interesting hypotheses that need to be further examined in order to better understand the continuity and discontinuity of different attachment patterns. Finally, Fonagy and colleagues have demonstrated that a parent’s capacity for understanding the mental states of self and others (which they term reflective-self function) is related to both maternal and infant security (Fonagy et al., 1991; Fonagy et 22 al., 1993). In order to test this, they created a Reflective-Self Function scale to be coded fi'om the AAI. As expected, autonomous mothers (as measured by the AAI during pregnancy) had significantly higher reflective-self scores than dismissive and preoccupied mothers, and mothers with higher reflective-self scores were also more likely to have securely attached infants at 1 year. Thus, a mother’s ability to anticipate her infant’s needs by being in tune with her infant’s mental world presumably leads to more sensitive caregiving and more secure attachment. Just as some mothers are aware of and in tune with their own and their infants’ internal world, other mothers are unaware of such internal experiences. Fonagy et al. (1993) reported that mothers’ incoherence in telling narratives about their childhood experiences on the AAI was the clearest predictor of infant insecurity 12 months later. The authors explained that incoherence was indicative of certain defenses, and indeed, there was also evidence of idealization, repression of affect, and splitting in these mothers’ interviews. The authors concluded that the defensive strategies used by some mothers may explain an unconscious mechanism underlying the transmission of insecure attachment. In other words, a mother’s own defenses against acknowledging certain experiences in her life make it difficult to respond empathically to her infant, which in turn, leads to the development of similar defensive strategies in the child. The studies conducted by Fonagy and colleagues provide strong empirical support for the clinical writings of Fraiberg et al. (1975), who noted early on that mother-child dyads fiequently and unknowingly repeat “scenes” fi'om the mothers’ own childhood experiences with caregivers. Moreover, studies by F onagy and colleagues support the notion of continuity of attachment across generations, especially for secure and dismissing/avoidant patterns. 23 The majority of studies examining the link between maternal representations of attachment and mother-infant attachment have been conducted with primarily Caucasian, well educated, and economically advantaged samples. This is problematic because of the limited generalizability of results and possible skewed distributions toward attachment security (including higher rates of concordance across generations due to the greater stability of security). However, two known studies have investigated the transmission of attachment in more high-risk samples (Levine et al., 1991; Ward & Carlson, 1995). In these studies, the AAI was administered during pregnancy to adolescent mothers (N = 42 and 72, respectively) from diverse, ethnic backgrounds (majority were Afi'ican-American) and later conducted the Strange Situation when infants were 15 months old. As expected, mothers had much higher rates of insecure representations of attachment (e.g., 21% autonomous, 48% dismissive, 7% preoccupied, 24% unresolved; Levine et al., 1991) compared to studies using low-risk samples. However, consistent with prior studies, results demonstrated a highly significant relationship between maternal representations of attachment and later mother-infant attachment. Levine et al. reported a 62% concordance rate using the four categories of attachment, and an 83% rate when all three insecure groups were combined for a two-group comparison, while Ward and Carlson (1995) reported a 78% concordance when using two-groups. Further analyses revealed a significant degree of concordance between autonomous mothers and secure infants (100% and 86%, respectively), dismissive mothers and avoidant infants (60% and 73%, respectively), and preoccupied mothers and ambivalent infants (66% and 60%, respectively). The studies did not find significant continuity of 24 unresolved/disorganized attachment across generations in these adolescent samples, with concordance rates of only 30% and 43%, respectively. Taken together, preliminary results fiom studies with high-risk samples (e. g., adolescent mothers) are consistent with results fi'om middle-class, well educated samples. They provide firrther support for the link between maternal attachment representations and mother-infant attachment as posited by attachment theory and clinical observations, as well as further evidence that there is relatively less stability (but still significant in some cases) among certain insecure groups. However, these results must be interpreted with caution because of the srmll sample sizes (e.g., only three mothers in the preoccupied group for one study). In addition, there is a clear need for more studies with different types of high-risk samples, i.e., low income or depressed mothers, since adolescent motherhood is qualitatively distinct from other risk factors. In sum, it appears that maternal representations of attachment are a strong predictor of the types of attachment mothers form with their own infants. According to a recent meta-analysis of 18 studies and 854 subjects (van IJzendoorn, 1995), the effect size of this relationship was 1.06, which is comparable to a concordance rate of 75% (kappa = .49), using secure and insecure classifications. Interestingly, when the unresolved group was not used in studies, the dismissing and preoccupied classifications both predicted their respective infant groups (combined gs = .45 and .42, respectively); however, the preoccupied classification was only marginally related to the ambivalent group in infancy when the unresolved group was included in analyses. van Ijzendoom (1995) also reported that only about 12% of the variation in maternal sensitivity, believed to be a possible mechanism through which attachment is transmitted, is explained by 25 maternal representations of attachment. Thus, other processes are likely occurring and further research needs to be conducted to more fully explain the continuities and discontinuities of attachment. That is, possible mechanisms through which attachment is (or is not) transmitted fiom one generation to the next must be measured and examined empirically, which unfortunately has been largely missing in the current literature. The few mechanisms that have been suggested include maternal sensitivity and responsiveness, the degree of disorganization among a mother’s internal working model which gives rise to contradictory behavior and fitulty communication (Bretherton, 1990), and a mother’s defensive strategies, evidenced by an inability to integrate past experiences and a tendency to distort current relationships, which may force an infant to rely on similar maladaptive defensive strategies (Cassidy & Kobak, 1988; Fonagy et al., 1993; Grossman et al., 1988; Main & Goldwyn, 1984b; Slade & Cohen, 1996). Other possible mechanisms of transmission that are consistent with attachment theory and appear to be promising explanations based on the available literature are rmtemal representations of the infant and representations of the self-as-mother. Just as working models of the self develop along with working models of others during childhood, representations of the infant and representations of the self-as-mother develop together during pregnancy. A number of attachment researchers have shown that these representations are similar in both content and organization (e.g., Ammaniti et al., 1992). That is, the way mothers conceptualize themselves as mothers is similar to the way mothers think about their infants. Although some researchers believe representations of selfand infant are distinct (Ammaniti et al., 1992; Stern, 1995), other attachment researchers conceptualize them as integrated “caregiving” representations 26 (George & Solomon, 1989, 1996). The current study proposes to examine representations of self-as-mother and infant as complementary but distinct, just as representations of self and other are complementary but distinct during childhood. Therefore, the possible role of each type of representation as a mediator between mothers’ own attachment and mother-infant attachment will be considered separately, and existing studies in each area will be reviewed next. Maternal Representations of the Infant Although not as prevalent as the literature on maternal representations of attachment to caregivers, some theorists and researchers have written about mothers’ representations of their infants, particularly during pregnancy. Bibring, Dwyer, Huntington, and Valenstein (1961) were among the first to discuss the psychological processes that occur during pregnancy including processes related to the “earliest mother- child relationship.” They noted that pregnancy seems to revive old psychological conflicts, reorganize the new mother’s relationship with her own mother, and causes the mother to develop attitudes and representations of her developing infant. As the infant begins to move and grow inside the mother, there is a shift fi'om a focus on the self (“enhanced narcissism”) to a focus on the infant as a separate object. Leifer (1977) also reported that women gradually develop rich and specific representations of their infants as pregnancy progresses, most often beginning in the second trimester, which prepares them psychologically for motherhood. Similarly, Lumley (1982) reported that women in their first trimester of pregnancy have difficulty imagining what the fetus might be like and are more focused on their own bodily changes, but they increasingly develop representations of their infants as the pregnancy progresses. For example, only 30% of 27 mothers in her sample considered the fetus “a person” in the first trimester, but 63% and 92% of women in the second and third trimesters perceived them as separate individuals, giving vivid descriptions of their babies. Some researchers in the health professions, such as nursing, have also described the process through which pregnant women begin to interact with their infants by developing ideas and feelings for the unborn children. The majority of these studies have assessed “prenatal attachment” through use of questionnaires and have found that attachment (defined here as positive feelings toward the infant) increases during pregnancy, with larger increases following quickening (see Muller, 1992 for a review). It has been suggested by these researchers that early feelings toward the infant form the basis for the mother’s relationship with her child after birth Zeanah and colleagues have examined mothers’ perceptions of their infants during pregnancy in a number of studies with both low- and high-risk groups of women. In one study, they administered infant temperament questionnaires and an interview about maternal perceptions to 35 pregnant, Caucasian women from middle-class backgrounds (Zeanah, Keener, & Anders, 1986a; Zeanah, Keener, Stewart, & Anders, 1985; Zeanah, Zeanah, & Stewart, 1990). They found that mothers had fairly stable perceptions of their infiants’ temperament throughout pregnancy, which also predicted their perceptions postnatally. They noted that women were able to give vivid, abstract descriptions of their babies’ personalities before birth, and perceptions during pregnancy seemed to be strikingly positive. These authors replicated their findings in a sample of adolescent, mostly Hispanic girls (N=24), by showing that maternal perceptions of infant temperament were significantly stable fiom 32 weeks to 36 weeks gestation to 1 month 28 postnatally (Zeanah, Keener, & Anders, 1986b; Zeanah, Keener, Anders, & Vieira-Baker, 1987). Zeanah et al. (1986b) concluded that adolescent mothers also develop detailed perceptions of their infants, which likely impact the developing relationships with their children. Similar to the work by Zeanah and colleagues, another group of researchers examined maternal perceptions of infants during pregnancy by administering an infant temperament questionnaire during the second and third trimesters, as well as postnatally, to 41 middle-class women (Mebert, 1989; Mebert & Kalinowski, 1986). Their results also indicated that mothers form stable perceptions of their infants during pregnancy and continuing after the baby is born. In addition, they found that first-time parents had significantly more negative views of their infants than multiparous parents. In a number of reports, Zeanah and colleagues (Zeanah et al., 1986b; Zeanah et al., 1987; Zeanah & Carr, 1990) indicated that fetal movements contribute to the formation of mothers’ initial perceptions of their infants which then develop into stable, detailed representations. Further, they suggested that fetal movements may act as a “projective stimulus,” from which mothers form elaborate representations that are not simply made up randomly, but likely constructed fiom the mothers’ own experiences in relationships. Since a mother’s own relationship with her mother appears to be reactivated during pregnancy, it seems probable that this particular relationship may have an impact on the mother’s developing representations of her infant. Although the studies by Zeanah et al. (1985, 1986a, 1986b, 1987) and Mebert et al. (1986, 1989) described above provide interesting information about mothers’ perceptions of their infants during pregnancy, they are limited by the sole focus on infant 29 temperament, the bias of self-report measures, and the atheoretical nature of their studies. For example, they do not address why mothers form vivid perceptions of their infants before birth, why these perceptions are so stable, or how mothers understand and experience their infants more generally. In contrast, more recent work on maternal ' perceptions during pregnancy has been guided by both attachment and psychoanalytic theories. In fact, Zeanah and colleagues were among the first to refiame earlier findings according to attachment theory (Zeanah & Anders, 1987; Zeanah & Barton, 1989), by suggesting that mothers form internal working models of their infants during pregnancy which, like other internal working models, are relatively stable and guide perceptions of their infants and behaviors toward them. Since that time, several groups of researchers have attempted to develop more theory-driven, qualitative measures to assess rmternal representations of infants. Maternal Rapresentations of Attachment and Representations of the Infant Ammaniti and colleagues (Ammaniti, 1991; Ammaniti et al., 1992) hypothesized ' that during pregnancy, representations of self and other are modified and reworked, particularly of self-as-mother and the “imaginary” child. Like others have suggested (Leifer, 1977; Zeanah et al., 1987), representational processes accelerate after the first 3 months when the infant becomes more subjectively real due to growth and movement. In order to investigate these psychological processes, they developed the Interview of Maternal Representations During Pregnancy (IRMA G), a semi-structured interview about parents’ reactions to and experiences of pregnancy, perceptions of their infants, and their own personal experiences during childhood. Similar to the AAI, they developed a coding system that emplmsized content-flee dimensions such as the coherence and 30 quality of the narrative (or how parents talked about their experiences). The authors administered this interview and several adjective checklists (asking mothers to rate themselves in various roles and their infants) to 23 middle-class, Italian women in their last trimester of pregnancy. They found that women developed rich representations of themselves as mothers and of their babies during pregnancy, and that individual differences existed between women. In addition, content-flee dimensions of their interviews (i.e., coherence, openness, richness) were highly related between representations of selfand of infimts. That is, the organization of women’s narratives was consistent across domains of self and other, as would be expected based on attachment theory. Women also tended to rate their infants similarly to themselves on a number of characteristics measured by adjective checklists (although they were significantly different on some characteristics like sociability and independence). Overall, the authors concluded that women develop clear representations of themselves and their infants during pregnancy, and that these are complementary in both organization and content. Further, through a series of case studies, Ammaniti (1991) indicated that maternal representations of self and infant during pregnancy were related to mothers’ own representations of attachment (according to the AAI) and to mother-infant attachment in the Strange Situation. These findings imply that prenatal representations may be one mechanism through which attachment is transmitted across generations, and it would be useful for future research to examine this with a larger, more heterogeneous sample. In a similar study (Fava Vizziello et al., 1993), 51 pregnant women from middle to upper-middle class backgrounds were administered an interview about representations 31 of the self, their infants, and their own childhood experiences, as well as asked to make ratings along a number of bipolar-adjective lists. The content and organization of women’s narratives produced during the interview were coded for several themes including: a) a desire to repair or compensate for previous experiences (not necessarily considered pathological), b) a defensive effort to maintain current fimctioning, c) intense fear stemming fiom matemal anxiety about the health and development of the child, and d) a lack of organization or consistent theme during the interview. First, in analyzing the stability of representations over time, the authors reported that representations of women’s own mothers were most stable, while representations of self-as-mother and of the infant were only somewhat stable fi-om pregnancy to postpartum (according to adjective ratings). During pregnancy, fear themes seemed to dominate women’s representations during interviews as compared to other themes; most mothers displayed conflicted fantasies of life and death and showed great concern for the babies’ health Women who displayed themes of a desire to repair or compensate or those unable to organize themes in the interview during pregnancy were significantly more likely to show similar themes after the birth Like the findings reported by Ammaniti et al. (1991 , 1992), these results suggest that women’s representations during pregnancy are detailed and rich and relatively stable in organization. In addition, F ava Vizziello et al. (1993) found that representations of attachment fiom childhood predicted mothers’ representations of themselves as mothers, but not representations of their infants. Again, it seems important to replicate these findings in other samples, as well as to validate these different interview techniques that are designed to assess maternal representations. 32 In an effort to better understand the role of prenatal representations in the transmission of attachment, Slade and Cohen (1996) developed and administered the Pregnancy Interview, a semi-structured interview for assessing women’s internal working models of their infants, to 66 married, middle-class women in their last trimester of pregnancy. They also administered the AAI during pregnancy to assess matemal representations of attachment. Although analyses fi‘om the total sample were unfortunately not presented, the authors described the results fi‘om a number of case studies, which revealed a strong association between mothers’ representations of attachment based on childhood experiences and their representations of their infants. Mothers tended to conceptualize and organize their relationship with their babies during pregnancy in ways that were similar to the way they conceptualized their relationships with their own mothers, evidenced by the degree of flexibility, coherence, and richness in their representations. In other words, mothers were beginning to repeat their own experiences during childhood with their infants (at the representational level) before the infants were even born. Although these findings are consistent with attachment theory and with previous findings (Ammaniti et al., 1992), the results should be interpreted with caution since they are based only on case studies using a different, unvalidated interview. Another study empirically examined the relationship between maternal representations of attachment and representations of children in 32 middle-class, Caucasian mothers (George & Solomon, 1996). When children were 6 years old, mothers were administered the AAI and the Experiences of Caregiving Interview, designed to assess mothers’ representations of themselves as parents and representations of their children (combined to form a “caregiving” representation). Scales were 33 developed to assess both the content and organization of mothers’ responses, which resulted in general categories that were analogous to the four AAI categories. Results revealed a significant concordance between representations of attachment and representations of caregiving (69% match with a kappa = .58). There was a 77% match between categories for the three traditional attachment groups (autonomous, dismissing, preoccupied). Although this study examined internal working models among mothers of older children rather than infants who are not yet born, it makes an important contribution to the literature by demonstrating a statistically significant relationship between mothers’ representations of attachment and representations of their children. Thus, it is important to assess these relationships among pregnant women and their infants in more diverse samples, in order to better understand how patterns of attachment are repeated through representational processes. Overall, the current body of literature shows an association, albeit preliminary, between mothers’ representations of attachment and representations of their own infants or children and also suggests that representations of the infant may be a mechanism through which attachment patterns are transmitted. Other researchers have focused on the second pathway in this process: the relationship between representations of the infant and mother-infant attachment. These studies will be reviewed next. Maternal Representations) ofth_e Infant and Mother-Infant Attachment In one such study, Bretherton et al. (1989) administered the Parent Attachment Interview, developed by the authors to assess mothers’ thoughts and feelings about their infants and their relationships with their infants, as well as the Strange Situation to mothers (N = 37) of 2 year olds. One year later, they also administered an attachment 34 story completion task to measure attachment at age 3. Results revealed that mothers who demonstrated sensitivity and insight into their relationships with their children based on interview responses (and therefore, presumably had more “secure” representations of their children) were more likely to have securely attached children at age 2 and age 3. Similarly, George and Solomon (1989, 1996) reported that mothers who displayed more secure representations of their 6 year-old children (defined as an understanding of the child’s needs, as well as coherent organization of thought processes related to these issues) had more securely attached children during a separation-reunion procedure. In fact, secure maternal representations accounted for more than 50% of the variance in child security. Furthermore, the concordance between classifications for caregiving representations and attachment classifications was highly significant at 81% (kappa = .75). The authors concluded that the way in which mothers thought about their children and their relationships with their children had a significant association with the attachment pattern their children developed. Although the results reported by Bretherton et al. (1989) and George and Solomon (1989, 1996) are consistent with attachment theory and provide empirical support for a link between maternal representations of caregiving and mother-child attachment, they are not necessarily generalizable to pregnant women or parents with young infants or to high-risk samples. In addition, like most other research in this area, they are limited by small samples and varying methods of assessment. Because each group of researchers tend to develop their own interviews to assess maternal representations of the child, it is difficult to compare results across studies because different constructs are being coded and analyzed. In addition, existing measures are not 35 being validated across different sarrrples, making it unclear how useful these measures are. In contrast, Zeanah and colleagues (Zeanah et al., 1994) have recently developed an interview that is being used more extensively, i.e., across different samples of both non-clinical and clinical groups, to measure mothers’ representations of their infants. The Working Model of the Child Interview (WMCI) is a 1 hour structured interview that inquires about a mother’s perceptions and subjective experiences of her infant and relationship with her infant. Interview responses are rated along a number of scales that assess qualitative (i.e., richness of representations, coherence, and Openness to change), content (i.e., infant difficulty), and affective features (i.e., joy, pride, anger) of maternal representations. Coders then assign an overall classification to the narratives provided by the mothers based on the scale profiles. This classification system is consistent with attachment theory and with AAI and Strange Situation categories. Balanced narratives include both positive and negative characteristics of the infant. They convey value for the infant’s individuality and appreciation for the infant’s subjective experience. The caregiver’s perceptions are open to change and are at least moderately rich in detail about the infant and the caregiving experiences. Disengaged representations are characterized by emotional distance or indifference towards the infant. Caregivers are unable to recognize the infant’s individuality, and if it is recognized, it is not valued. Details about the infant or parenting experience lack richness, are not flexible or open to new experience, and are emotionally unintegrated. Finally, Distorted representations reflect general inconsistencies. Caregivers rmy be preoccupied or overwhelmed by the infant or may have unrealistic expectations about the 36 infant. Unlike disengaged parents, distorted parents do not deny their impact on their infants. However, they Often do not recognize how their behaviors may be detrimental to the infants. Much feeling is expressed toward the infants, but these emotions lack a sense of modulation or meaning. In their original study, Zeanah et al. (1994) administered the WMCI to middle- class mothers of 12-month old infants (N = 45), as well as the Strange Situation to all mother-infant dyads. They found that WMCI categories were significantly related to Strange Situation categories, with an overall concordance rate of 69% (kappa = .50). Seventy-four percent of mothers with balanced representations of their infants had secure attachments according to the Strange Situation, while 73% of disengaged mothers had avoidant infants and 55% of distorted mothers had ambivalent infants. In addition, mothers of secure infants scored significantly higher on a number of WMCI subscales including richness of perceptions, openness to change, coherence, and caregiving sensitivity than mothers of insecure infants. Consistent with other interviews such as the AAI, content-free dimensions were especially important for distinguishing mothers of secure and insecure infants. In a later study, these researchers administered the WMCI to 96 Caucasian, upper- middle class pregnant women and then readministered the WMCI along with the Strange Situation when infants were approximately 1 year old (Benoit, Parker, & Zeanah, 1997). First, they reported that WMCI categories were significantly stable fiom pregnancy to postpartum (80% agreement, kappa = .59), although this was accounted for by the stability of balanced (89%) and distorted (85%) mothers. The strong stability of working models over time is consistent with attachment theory, although results also suggested 37 that certain types of working models may be less stable across time. Results also revealed a 73% concordance rate between concurrent WMCI and Strange Situation categories, including an 88% match for balanced-secure groups, 100% match for disengaged-avoidant groups, but only 40% match for distorted-ambivalent groups. Similar to Zeanah et al. (1994), the high overall concordance was primarily due to the balanced and disengaged groups. Perhaps most interestingly, Benoit et al. (1997) reported a significant concordance between mothers’ representations of their infants during pregnancy and Strange Situation classifications at 1 year (overall match rate = 74%, kappa = .44), although this association was rminly explained by the balanced-secure groups (91% concordance) rather than the disengaged (30% concordance) and distorted groups (50%). One possible explanation for the lower concordance rates for disengaged and distorted groups is that continuity may not be specific to type of insecurity. That is, disengaged mothers may have ambivalent infants and vice versa Therefore, it may be more useful to look at “secure” and “insecure” groups. Because the WMCI classifies representations into three categories (rather than four like the AAI and Strange Situation), studies using the WMCI are only able to compare classifications with the three original Strange Situation categories. Therefore, another possibility is that infants who might have been classified as disorganized on the Strange Situation were “forced” into one of the other categories, confounding the results for those groups. Still, because the WMCI was administered prenatally rather than concurrently with the Strange Situation, the findings indicate that prenatal representations might influence the way in which mothers’ perceive and interact with their children after they 38 are born, which in turn could impact the type of attachment formed. It will be important for more studies to use prospective designs such as this one to better understand the processes involved in the transmission Of attachment. In addition, the relationship between WMCI and Strange Situation categories should be examined in more diverse and larger size samples. Overall, the results of studies using the WMCI indicate its usefulness as a tool for measuring matemal representations of infants, as well as for predicting mother-infant attachment. In sum, a considerable amount of literature suggests that mothers develop internal representations of their infants well before birth and lasting into childhood. These representations, or internal working models, of infants appear to be influenced by mothers’ representations of attachment based on their own childhood experiences. That is, mothers tend to think about and conceptualize their infants and the relationship they have with their infants in similar ways to how they think about their relationships with caregivers. Furthermore, a number of studies have indicated that maternal representations of infants, both before and after birth, predict the quality of mother-infant attachment. Thus, it seems possible that mothers’ representations of their infants is one mechanism through which mothers’ own attachment impacts the quality of attachment they form with their own infants. Another possible mechanism, as mentioned earlier, is maternal representations of self-as-mother. Mothers’ representations of attachment may influence representations of themselves, which my in turn impact the attachment formed with their infants. Studies that have examined the relationship between mothers’ perceptions of themselves and mothers’ own attachment experiences and their attachment with their infants will be 39 reviewed next, beginning with a brief theoretical introduction about representations of the self. Maternal Representations of Self-As-Mother As mentioned previously, because internal working models of self and others develop together in the context of early interactions with caregivers, they tend to be complementary (Bowlby, 1973; Bretherton, 1990; Bretherton & Munholland, 1999; Slade & Cohen, 1996). Therefore, a working model of the self as valuable and competent will develop in the context of a caregiver who treats the infant as valuable, i.e., who is emotionally available and sensitive. In contrast, a working model of the self as incompetent and unlovable will develop in the context of a rejecting or unempathic parent. This notion of complementarity has received empirical support; two studies to date have demonstrated that securely attached children are more likely to develop a positive working model of self, while insecurely attached children develop either a negative working model of self or idealized, distorted self-representations (Cassidy, 1988; Verschueren, Marcoen, & Schoefs, 1996). More recently, attachment theorists have questioned whether this early sense of self determines one’s global self-worth (in multiple areas and relationships), as attachment theory would suggest, or whether it determines one’s working model of the self in relation to the attachment figure (Cassidy, 1990). Like attachment theory, Epstein’s self-theory states that individuals develop beliefs about the self early in life that are more global, i.e., “I am worthy of love,” and these beliefs are based in large part on interactions with caregivers (Epstein, 1973; Ricks, 1985). Because new information is assimilated into these global representations, they tend to be long lasting and resistant to 40 change. In contrast, other theories such as Bandura’s self-efficacy theory (1977, 1989) propose that beliefs about oneself are situation-specific and change in response to different demands, situations, and individual developmental processes. In the current study, representations of the self and self-efficacy beliefs will be considered separate constructs, although more general representations of the self are thought to influence self-efficacy beliefs more or less in different situations and roles. Because this study is examining representations of self-as-mother and self-efficacy in the caregiving role specifically, it is expected that representations will be strongly related to self-eficacy beliefs. Mammal Representa_ti_ons of Attachment and Representations of Self-As-Mother A number of theorists and researchers have proposed that during pregnancy, women begin to modify and rework representations of themselves as they prepare for motherhood. Stern (1995) stated that a mother’s representations of herself as a woman, wife, daughter, career-person, and in other roles are activated and reorganized as she makes room for her role as a mother. Consistent with attachment theory, Rubin (1984) also suggested that women go through a process of redefining themselves as they form a “maternal identity.” This is achieved when the woman sees herself in relation to her unborn child and is considered important to the woman’s sense of competency in being a mother to her child. As previously mentioned, several groups of researchers have indeed demonstrated that pregnant women develop vivid representations of themselves as mothers throughout pregnancy and these representations are fairly stable over time (Ammaniti, 1991; Ammaniti et al., 1992; Slade & Cohen, 1996). 41 Within the fiamework of attachment theory, preliminary evidence also suggests that mothers’ representations of attachment based on experiences during childhood influence the way in which they see themselves as mothers. That is, internal working models developed early in life contribute to or guide the development of internal working models of the self as mother. For example, several previously mentioned studies have reported that mothers’ representations of their own attachment experiences with their mothers were highly related to their representations of themselves as mothers (Amrmniti, 1991; Ammaniti et al., 1992; Fava Vizziello et al., 1993). Several other studies also reported that mothers’ representations of attachment according to the AAI were related to representations of themselves; mothers who had more secure representations tended to see themselves in more positive ways and were more flexible and coherent when describing themselves as mothers, while insecure mothers described themselves more negatively and had more incoherent, distorted representations of themselves as mothers (George & Solomon, 1996; Slade & Cohen, 1996). Another study also investigated the relationship between mothers’ experiences with caregivers and their views of themselves as mothers (Meyer, 1988). Questionnaires assessing childhood experiences and parenting efficacy were administered to 35 well- educated, middle class mothers with infants who were approximately 14 months old. Results revealed that positive experiences with caregivers during childhood were strongly related to positive views of themselves as mothers and higher maternal self-efficacy. Thus, mothers who reported having more caring and sensitive mothers were more likely to see themelves as caring and sensitive and felt more competent in caring for their infants compared to women who reported negative experiences with their mothers. It 42 should be noted that experiences with mothers during childhood were particularly important for maternal perceptions of eficacy compared with experiences with fathers, which were unrelated to the way mothers viewed themselves. Unfortunately, this study did not assess current mother-child attachment so it is unclear how maternal feelings of efficacy were related to mothers’ relationships with their infants in this sample. M_aieLn_al Representations of Self As Moth}; and Mother-Infant Attachment In addition to those studies showing a relationship between mothers’ experiences during childhood with attachment figures and their representations of themselves, a number of other studies have investigated the link between mothers’ representations of themselves as mothers and their current attachment with their infants. Through several case studies, Ammaniti (1991) suggested that mothers’ representations of themselves as mothers were related to mother-infant attachment observed during the Strange Situation. As expected, mothers who felt more confident and positive about their ability to parent were more likely to have infants who were securely attached. Similarly, George and Solomon (1989, 1996) reported that mothers who displayed more secure representations of themselves as mothers (characterized by a willingness and ability to provide safety and protection, a positive evaluation of abilities, and little self-doubt or confirsion over caretaking) had more securely attached children during a separation-reunion procedure. Thus, these studies suggest that another possible mechanism through which mothers’ own attachment impacts infant attachment is mothers’ representations of themselves as (in)competent, (in)efl“ective mothers. That is, representations of attachment fi'om childhood may influence the development of working models of the self-as-mother, which in turn, impact the attachment that is formed with a mother’s own infant. 43 M_at_e__rnal Self-Efficacy and Mothgr-Infant Attachment Although only a few studies have examined the impact of women’s representations of themselves on mother-infant attachment through interviews designed to measure representations per se, a number of other studies have looked at the impact of maternal self-efficacy on mother-infant relationships and specifically, mother-infant attachment. It is believed that maternal self-eficacy, defined as a mother’s perceived ability to perform competently and effectively as a mother, is largely influenced by a woman’s representhtions of the self as mother. Therefore, it is argued that maternal self- eflicacy is directly related to representations of the self and is also likely related to mother-infant attachment. One study examined maternal self-efficacy, “attachment” (defined as maternal feelings toward her infant), and overall self-esteem and depression in 121 medically high- risk and 182 low-risk women during and after their pregnancies (Mercer & F erketick, 1990). Results revealed that self-efficacy was the best predictor of attachment for both groups of women; women who felt more capable of providing competent care felt closer to and more involved with their infants. Similarly, Mercer and F erketick (1994) reported that self-efficacy was strongly related to experienced mothers’ attachment to their infants, but they reported no such relationship for inexperienced mothers. In another study, 238 women were interviewed during their last trimester of pregnancy and part of the sample was again interviewed at 1 month postpartum (N = 86) and 2 years postpartum (N = 62; Williams et al., 1987). Results from this study revealed that self-efficacy measured prenatally and at 1 month predicted attachment at 1 month only, while self-efficacy at 2 years predicted the quality of the mother-child relationship at 2 years according to rmternal report. Unfortunately, all of these studies used maternal report questionnaires to assess “attachment” at different ages. It is likely that these instrmnents were measuring maternal attitudes and perceptions of the mother-child relationship rather than attachment behaviors and patterns as described by attachment theory. Thus, one can only conclude fi'om these studies that maternal feelings of self-efficacy likely impact the way in which mothers view their relationships with their infants. Only a few studies have actually assessed the relationship between self-efficacy and mother-infant attachment as measured by the Strange Situation. In one study, Spieker and Booth (1988) examined several potential predictors of mother-infant attachment in 35 high-risk (defined by few resources and low levels of support) mother- infant dyads. Questionnaires were administered to mothers prenatally and at 6 weeks and 3 months postnatally, and the Strange Situation was administered when children were 14 months old. The authors found that mothers of secure infants reported more confidence handling motherhood tasks shortly after birth, and therefore considered themselves more efficacious, than mothers of ambivalent infants. Self-efficacy ratings did not differ among mothers of secure and avoidant infants; however, the authors suggested that mothers of avoidant children might have been defensively reporting unrealistically high levels of self-efficacy (consistent with other defensive processes observed in mothers of avoidant infants). Although this study had a small sample size, the prospective design and high-risk nature of the sample make it an important and unique contribution to the literature. More studies need to be done to better understand how mothers’ views of themselves and their maternal competence impact mother-infant attachment. 45 Donovan and colleagues (Donovan & Leavitt, 1989; Donovan, Leavitt, & Walsh, 1990) also measured maternal self-efficacy (through the attributional styles mothers used to describe the outcomes of their caregiving) in 40 white, married women with infants. In addition, the authors measured mothers’ perceived control in an experimental paradigm. During this procedure, mothers listened to a tape-recorded infant cry and were given different options for responding to the crying. They were told that the procedure was a simulated child-care task and that after hearing the infant cry, they could choose whether or not to press a red button. Afterwards, mothers were asked to estimate how much control they thought they had over the termination of the cry. Because neither response was more effective than the other in reality, any perceived control that mothers reported was considered “illusion of control.” Approximately one year later, the researchers also administered the Strange Situation to all mother-infant dyads. The results showed that low self-efficacy was related to a high illusion of control. Furthermore, mothers of securely attached children reported higher self-eflicacy and lower illusions of control (therefore, they were more realistic about their abilities), while mothers of insecurely attached children reported lower self-efficacy and higher illusions of control. The authors concluded that high illusory control may be a defensive strategy used by mothers to mask feelings of inefiicacy. Thus, it seems important to distinguish between mothers who are realistically self-efficacious and those that are unrealistic in their perceptions of efficacy. The latter group may be at particular risk of parenting problems because their coping mechanism of illusory control may prevent them from acknowledging and improving their own limitations. 46 Because the quality of mother-infant interactions and specific maternal behaviors are the main contributors to specific patterns of attachment, it is important to note that a number of studies have also found a relationship between maternal self-efficacy and more sensitive, positive maternal behaviors. For example, several studies have reported that mothers who felt more competent and efficacious in the maternal role were actually more sensitive and responsive when interacting with their infants (Shea & Tronick, 1988; Teti & Gelfand, 1991). This relationship seems to be especially strong for first-time mothers, who do not have a previously established maternal identity (Walker, Crain, & Thompson, 1986). Conrad, Gross, Fogg, and Ruchala (1992) also found that mothers who were more confident in their parenting abilities demonstrated more positive interactions with their toddlers, although this was true only for mothers who were knowledgeable about child development. The authors concluded that accurate knowledge and confidence (or self- efficacy) were both important for the quality of maternal behaviors towards their children. Overall, most existing studies have suggested a link between self-efficacy and more positive maternal behaviors (with some exceptions, e.g., Zahr, 1991), but results need to be replicated since existing samples are small and consist mainly of white, middle-class, educated individuals. In conclusion, the existing research suggests that mothers’ representations of themselves, and in particular of themselves as mothers, are influenced by experiences they had with their attachment figures during childhood. More specifically, mothers who were sensitively cared for by their own mothers are more likely to view themselves as competent, caring mothers to their children, while mothers who had more problematic attachment relationships are less likely to feel confident in their abilities to parent. 47 Further, existing studies indicate that maternal perceptions and feelings of efficacy impact the quality of mother-child interactions and more importantly, the types of attachment they form with their infants. Therefore, it seems probable that another mechanism through which attachment patterns are repeated across generations is through mothers’ representations of themselves as mothers. Overall, it is likely that individual, intrapsychic factors like those that have been described thus far (e.g., representations of self and infant) have a considerable impact on the quality of attachment formed between a mother and her infant. However, it is probable that attachment is also influenced by external factors within the mother’s and infant’s environment. Therefore, the potential relationships between several ecological variables and mother-infant attachment will be reviewed next. 48 CHAPTER 3: ECOLOGICAL FACTORS RELATED TO ATTACHMENT The impact of a number of ecological factors on attachment has been considered by attachment researchers, including factors at the individual, family, and community levels. For example, some researchers have examined the relationship between child maltreatment and mother-infant attachment (e.g., Crittenden, 1985), while other researchers have looked at more global factors such as socioeconomic status (e.g., Spieker & Booth, 1988). Therefore, studies which have investigated the relationship between different ecological factors and mother-infant attachment will be reviewed, beginning with the literature on maternal social support and attachment, followed by a review of the literature on several risk factors and attachment. Maternal Social Support and Mother-Infant Attachment One possible environmental factor related to mother-infant attachment is the amount and type of social support available to mothers. According to Stern’s theory (1995), mothers need to create and maintain a support network so that they can accomplish the goals of motherhood; the support network bufl‘ers the mother fi'om external demands, protects her physically, provides emotional and practical support, as well as instruction and advice. In all of these ways then, social support can have an indirect, beneficial effect on mother-infant attachment. Some researchers have examined the impact of social support on attachment through an ecological model, assuming that factors at multiple system levels are important for explaining this aspect of the mother-infant relationship. In a review on social support and parenting, Crockenberg (1988) hypothesized various ways that social support can positively impact the parent-child relationship that are similar to those 49 mentioned by Stern (1995) including: a) reducing the number of stressful events that parents experience, b) buffering the effects of existing stressors, c) serving as a source of knowledge and competence, and (1) serving as a model for positive, nurturing relationships which parents may then repeat with their own children. Crockenberg (1988) concluded that in some or all of these ways, social support contributes to more sensitive, responsive parenting and presumably to more secure patterns of attachment between mothers and infants. Several empirical studies that have looked specifically at the impact of social support on mother-infant attachment provide support for the general assertions made by Stern (1995) and Crockenberg (1988). In one of the earliest studies of this kind, Crockenberg (1981) assessed the amount of perceived social support in the maternal role at 3 months postpartum in 48 mothers fi'om intact, middle-class families and later assessed attachment in the Strange Situation at 12 months. Results showed that the infants of mothers with more social support displayed significantly fewer anxious behaviors in the Strange Situation. In addition, there was an interaction between infant irritability and maternal support; mothers with low social support were more likely to have insecure infants, but this was only true for babies rated (by mothers) as temperamentally “difficult.” Thus, it seemed that low social support had a greater impact on the attachment with irritable babies than with “e ” babies, suggesting the importance of support in more stressed families. In a number of other studies, the impact of social support on attachment has been examined in high-risk samples. In one longitudinal study (Spieker, 1986; Spieker & Booth, 1988), mothers who were defined as high-risk because of reportedly little social 50 support were followed from pregnancy through the infants’ first year of life in order to determine contributors to parenting styles and quality of attachment. These researchers found that mothers of infants classified as having a disorganized attachment (believed to be “most” insecure) had significantly less support around the parenting role during pregnancy than mothers of infants classified as avoidant and had significantly less postpartum social support than mothers of securely attached infants. There were no other significant group differences on maternal social support during pregnancy or postpartum, possibly due to a lack of variance in level of support because of the inclusion criterion for this study. Unfortunately, this study did not look at possible mechanisms through which support might have afi‘ected attachment. In another longitudinal study, Crnic, Greenberg, and Slough (1986) followed 36 mother-premature infant dyads and found that maternal support from partners, fiiends, and the community at 1 month postpartum predicted secure mother-infant attachment at 12 months as assessed by a modified Strange Situation (a shortened version with one separation and one reunion). In addition, life stress was significantly related to insecure infant attachment. Although social support buffered the effects of stress on some outcome measures (e. g., parenting satisfaction), this was not true for attachment. Overall, the findings from these two longitudinal studies appear to confirm the importance of looking at ecological variables such as social support when investigating factors related to mother-infant attachment. However, like those with low-risk samples, these studies are generally limited by their small sample sizes. Using a larger sample size (N = 121), Crittenden (1985) evaluated the relationship between social support and attachment among low-income families, many of whom were 51 identified as abusive and/or neglectfirl by Child Protective Services. Regression analyses revealed that social support did not make a significant contribution to the explained variance in infant attachment after maltreatment status was accounted for (which explained 35% of the variance). When maltreatment status was not considered, Crittenden reported that the type or quality of support was related to mother-infant attachment security, while the quantity of social support (both the number of network members and total amount of support) was unrelated to attachment. More specifically, mothers who had stable, long-lasting relationships and who were fi'equently in contact with supporters had more securely attached infants, while mothers who had temporary, short-term relationships tended to have infants who were insecurely attached. The author interpreted these findings using attachment theory by suggesting that mothers’ working models of relationships affected both the pattern of attachment formed with their infants and the types of relationships formed with other adults who may (or my not) serve as sources of support. That is. the relationship between social support and mother-infant attachment might be a result of a third variable, the mother’s internal working models. This study illustrates the importance of looking at different dimensions of social support including the network size, level of support, and duration and frequency of contact with supporters. as each dimension may be differentially related or unrelated to the quality of mother-infant attachment. Like previously mentioned studies, this study also suggests that social support does not necessarily have a clear, direct relationship with mother-infant attachment. Instead, the impact of social support on attachment may be more indirect, i.e., through maternal attitudes or behaviors, and/or may vary according to the nature of the sample being studied. 52 Although the studies described thus far suggest that social support, in one way or another, has a significant effect on mother-infant attachment, other studies suggest that social support is unrelated to the quality of attachment. For example, Belsky and Isabella (1988) followed 64 Caucasian, intact families beginning in pregnancy through the infants’ first year of life in order to examine the determinants of infant attachment. They found that mothers of secure, avoidant, and ambivalent infants did not differ in their level of support fiom fiiends and family during pregnancy or postpartum, although mothers of secure babies perceived their neighborhoods (or “social milieu”) as friendlier and more supportive during pregnancy than mothers of insecure babies. Similarly, Zeanah et al. (1993) reported no differences between attachment groups on rmternal social support at 1 year postpartum in a middle-class sample. The results fi'om another study with a relatively small (N = 43) middle-class, intact sample (Levitt, Weber, & Clark, 1986) also revealed no group differences in emotional or parenting support. In the latter study, however, the authors reported that low support was related to negative maternal effect, which was related to attachment quality, suggesting a possible indirect effect of social support. Interestingly, the studies which have concluded that no relationship exists between social support and attachment have all examined these variables in low-risk samples. It is possible that social support is less critical for mothers and infants who have other resources and who have less general life stress than those families in high-risk situations who have fewer resources and more stress. There are a number of other possible reasons for the inconsistent results regarding the relationship between maternal social support and mother-infant attachment. One possible expLanation is that studies use different definitions of social support and most 53 fail to specify what dimensions of support are being measured including the type and source of support, the quantity of support or number of network members, as well as the fiequency of contact or duration of supportive relationships. As already mentioned, it is also possible that social support has a more indirect impact on mother-infant attachment through its impact on other factors, which are left unexamined in most existing studies. As suggested by Crockenberg (1988), one possible mechanism through which social support influences mother-infant attachment is through a mother’s feelings of self- efiicacy in the maternal role. Crockenberg and others (Bandura, 1977; Leifer, 1977; Stern, 1995) reason that support can help a mother increase her parenting skills and knowledge, which in turn increase feelings of eflicacy, can provide direct confirmation that she is capable and competent in caring for her infant, and can provide positive role models fiom which to learn. Indeed, several empirical studies lmve shown a significant relationship between maternal social support and maternal self-eficacy in both low and highrisk samples of women (Cutrona & Troutman, 1986; Shea & Tronick, 1988; Teti, O’Connell, & Reiner, 1996; Zahr, 1991). However, very few studies have examined the role of self-eficacy as a mechanism through which social support impacts attachment. In one related study, Donovan and Leavitt (1989) showed that women who demonstrated a high “illusion” of control around parenting (in an experimental paradigm) due to feelings of ineflicacy also reported low levels of social support and had infants who were more insecurely attached. Unfortunately, although ineflicacy was related to both social support and attachment, the authors did not statistically test for mediation. Isabella (1994) reported that although social support was not directly related to security of attachment in 32 mother-infant dyads, maternal support had an indirect impact on 54 attachment through its impact on maternal role satisfaction, a construct presumed to be highly related to self-efficacy (e.g., Shea & Tronick, 1988). That is, support was related to higher satisfaction, which was in turn related to attachment security. Finally, Teti and Gelfand (1991) demonstrated that self-efficacy mediated the relationship between social support and sensitive maternal behaviors, which underlie secure attachment, in mothers of infants between the ages of 3 and 13 months. In conclusion, it is still somewhat unclear if and how maternal social support affects the quality of mother-infant attachment. Existing results indicate that some dimensions or types of social support do influence attachment, however, it may be most appropriate to view social support as having an indirect impact on attachment rather than a direct impact (Belsky, 1999). As noted above, one mechanism through which social support may influence attachment is maternal self-eflicacy, although there is currently little empirical work in this area. The literature on social support and parenting behaviors (rather than attachment per se) is somewhat more voluminous, and several reviews (Andersen & Telleen, 1992; Crockenberg, 1988) in this area suggest that: a) social support is related to more sensitive caregiving, b) emotional support and support with child care tasks are particularly important types of support for mothers, c) social support from family members is especially important compared to other sources of support, and d) mothers’ perceived level of support is more important than the sheer number of supporters in her network. Because parenting behaviors and attachment are highly interrelated, it seems possible that these findings could help inform firture studies examining the role of support on mother-infant attachment. 55 Risk Factors and Mother-Infant Attachment In addition to maternal social support, the relationship between other ecological variables and mother-infant attachment has been examined by a number of researchers. For example, several potential enviromnental risk factors have been shown to negatively impact the quality of mother-infant attachment, indicating the need to consider risk factors in any study of attachment. In general, research has shown that there is a greater proportion of insecure attachment within high-risk samples (Spieker & Booth, 1988), as well as less stability of attachment quality over time (V ondra, Hommerding, & Shaw, 1999). Furthermore, research suggests that although a single risk factor may not distinguish secure fiom insecure infants, a clear and definite relationship between risk and attachment emerges when multiple risk factors are considered simultaneously (Belsky, 1999). Specifically, the more risk factors a Emily experiences, the more likely it is that mother-infant attachment will be insecure in one form or another. This is consistent with the cumulative-risk model described by Samerofl' and colleagues (Samerofl‘, Seifer, Barocas, Zax, & Greenspan, 1987), as well as other researchers (Harman & Luster, 1991; Luster & McAdoo, 1994), in that the number of risk factors may be more important for developmental outcomes in children than the specificity of risk factors. Unlike the majority of studies on mother-infant attachment, which have been based on stable, intact, middle-class families, the current study examined factors related to mother-infant attachment in a more diverse, high-risk sample. More specifically, almost half of the women in the current study experienced domestic violence during their current pregnancy, and a large proportion of women were economically disadvantaged. 56 Very few studies have examined patterns of attachment across generations in high-risk samples, making it unclear whether the processes suggested by attachment theory are the same for both low and high-risk families. Therefore, a number of important and relevant risk factors were considered in the current study. The existing literature on the relationship between these risk factors and mother-infant attachment is reviewed next. DomLestic Violgceand Adm The literature on domestic violence, defined as male to female violence between adult partners, and mother-inEnt attachment is sparse. However, research by Lyons-Ruth and colleagues (e.g., Lyons-Ruth et al., 1999; Lyons-Ruth & Jacobvitz, 1999) have suggested that mothers who are traumatized by past loss and/or experiences of abuse tend to show fiightening or fiightened caregiving behaviors, such as threatening gestures, unusual voice intonations, trance-like states, and fearful withdrawal fi'om their infants. These behaviors, in turn, have been related to disorganized infant attachment. Although research has focused on maternal experiences of loss and abuse during childhood specifically, it seems probable that traumatizing experiences of abuse during adulthood with intirrmte partners might have the same efl‘ect on maternal behaviors and mother- infant attachment. In the only empirical study to date, Zeanah et al. (1999) examined the relationship between domestic violence and mother-infant attachment in 72 low-income, mostly unrmrried mothers and their 15-month old infants. They found that mothers who had not experienced domestic violence were significantly more likely to have securely attached children, while mothers who were currently in an abusive relationship were significamly more likely to have infants with insecure attachments, particularly disorganized 57 attachments. These results are consistent with the notion that mothers traumatized by abusive experiences are more likely to develop insecure patterns of attachment with their infants. However, more studies need to be done to replicate these findings before strong conclusions can be made. Child Maltreatment and Attachment Because child abuse occurs in about 35% to 70% of all homes characterized by domestic violence (Jouriles & Norwood, 1995; O’Keefe, 1995), the impact of abuse on attachment was also considered. In fact, child maltreatment is perhaps the most widely studied risk factor in relation to mother-infant attachment. Child maltreatment has repeatedly been shown to be highly related to the quality of mother-infant attachment, with infants from all maltreatment groups being more likely to develop insecure attachments (Crittenden, 1985; Crittenden, 1988; Egeland & Sroufe, 1981; Erickson, Egeland, & Pianta, 1989; Lyons-Ruth, Connell, Grunebaurn, Botein, & 2011, 1984; Lyons-Ruth, Connell, Zoll, & Stahl, 1987). Furthermore, maltreated infants are particularly at risk for developing disorganized attachments, which show marked stability through the toddler years, compared to other forms of insecure attachment (Barnett, Gamban, & Cicchetti, 1999). Mal Depression an_d Attaq__hmgrt Maternal depression was also examined in the current study because battered women have repeatedly been shown to experience significantly more depression than non-battered women (Cascardi & O’Leary, 1992; Levendosky & Graham-Bermann, 2001 ). The impact of nmterml depression, an indicator of maternal psychological firnctioning, on mother-infant attachment has been investigated by several groups of 58 researchers. The overwhelming evidence indicates that depressed mothers are more likely to have insecurely attached infants than non-depressed mothers (Donovan & Leavitt, 1989; Radke-Yarrow, Cummings, Kuczynski, & Chapman, 1985; Spieker & Booth, 1988; Teti, Gelfand, Messinger, & Isabella, 1995), a finding that is not particularly surprising in light of the research that shows a strong relationship between matemal depression and unavailable, unresponsive, and/or rejecting maternal behaviors (see Gelfand & Teti, 1990 for a review). Interestingly, based on a review of studies examining the relationship between depression and infant attachment, Belsky (1999) concluded that the severity and chronicity of exposure to maternal depression is also an important determinant of insecure attachment. Not only will infants who are exposed to a depressed mother be more likely to have an insecure attachment than infants never exposed to maternal depression, but infants who have more severely depressed mothers and/or mothers who are depressed for longer periods of time will be even more likely to display an insecure attachment than infants who have depressed mothers of lesser severity and/or duration. Demoggaphic Risk and Attachment Low income and socioeconomic status (SES) have also repeatedly been linked to problems in parenting and caregiving behaviors (for a review, see Halpern, 1993). Because poverty produces a number of stressors on parents such as financial strain and unsafe and overcrowded communities, it tends to undermine parents’ well-being, feelings of control and competence, and abilities to adequately care for children’s physical and emotional needs. Poverty has been associated with less consistent, attentive caregiving, less emphasis on infant needs compared to personal or familial needs, parental 59 unavailability, and less responsiveness to infant emotional well-being, among other things (Bromwich, 1978; Norton, 1990). Overall, poverty and associated stressors tend to preoccupy parents in ways that interfere with their attentiveness to parenting and “preoccupation” with their infants (Halpern, 1993). Because low SES and income have been associated with these caregiving problems, it is likely that these risk factors impact the quality of mother-infant attachment as well. Although fewer studies have been conducted in this specific area, there is some evidence to support a significant relationship between lower economic status and insecure infant attachment, particularly fiom the literature showing a link between maternal life stress and attachment (e.g., Crnic et al. 1986). Although some large-scale studies have reported no relationship between SES and mother-infant attachment (e.g., Spieker & Booth, 1988), this is likely due to the low variance in SES in these samples. That is, virtually all attachment studies have been conducted with an exclusively middle- class sample or exclusively lower-class sample, rather than samples with a range of economic classes, which makes it statistically difficult to find a relationship between SES and attachment, even if one actually exists. Finally, because there is a large overlap between poverty and single parenthood (Halpern, 1993) and because single parenthood is related to a number of similar stressors, it is possible that single parenthood is related to the quality of mother-infant attachment. However, this relationship has yet to be investigated by attachment researchers. Although single parenthood and poverty are highly related, it seems important to consider single parenthood as an additional risk Ector since Samerofl‘ et al. (1987) suggest that the number of risk factors is more important than the specificity of risk. 60 CHAPTER 4: HYPOTHESES OF THE PRESENT STUDY As mentioned previously, Daniel Stern (1995) has recently developed a theory to explain the process that women go through as they prepare for motherhood and as they form an attachment with their infants after birth Much of Stern’s theory has been based on the attachment literature reviewed in this paper, as well as extensive clinical experience with mothers and infants. According to Stern, a mother’s mental representations of self and others (“networks of schemas-Of-being-with”) are reactivated and reworked throughout pregnancy, especially after the first trimester when the baby becomes more “real” to the mother. In particular, Stern indicates tint three sets of representations are especially important for a woman during this time. First, Stern (1995) suggests that a new mother’s relationship with her own mother becomes reactivated (consciously or unconsciously) during pregnancy as the new mother forms her own ideas about caregiving. It is believed that preparation for motherhood and the daily acts of mothering provide a specific remembering context, in which memories from the mother’s own childhood are evoked. Stern also asserts that a mother’s experiences of being mothered is a major influence on the way in which she interacts withherinfant andonthe quality ofattachment formed withthe infant, andhis conclusions are largely based on the empirical literature showing a significant relationship between mothers’ representations of their own attachment with caregivers and the attachment formed with their infants. During pregnancy, a mother also develops representations of her baby. Stern concludes fiom the available literature that there is an increase in richness and specificity of maternal representations of her fetus-as-inEnt around 4 months gestation, possibly 61 precipitated by the baby’s movement and growth. Although Stern suggests that representations may decline during the last trimester of pregnancy in order to protect the mother and baby from a potential discrepancy between the real and imagined baby, existing studies actually indicate that women continue to have richly developed representations during their entire pregnancy, as well as postpartum (e. g., Fava Vizziello et al., 1993; Leifer, 1977). In addition, a number of studies indicate that representations of the infant may be a mechanism through which attachment is transmitted fiom mother to infant (e.g., Ammaniti, 1991). Finally, another important representation during a woman’s transition to motherhood is her representation of self as a mother. Stern (1995) hypothesizes that an important internal transformation takes place, as the mother shifts from being a daughter to her mother to being a mother to her daughter/son. As part of this transformation, a mother’s representations of herself in various roles (e.g., wife, daughter, and mother) will be activated and reorganized as she makes psychological room for her role as a mother. Some empirical literature has indicated, in fact, that representations of self as mother and beliefs about caregiving efficacy are related to both representations of attachment and mother-infant attachment. Stern (1995) also asserts that there are a number of important themes within the mother’s psychological and external world as she makes the transition to motherhood. First, the mother is concerned with helping the infant grow and thrive physically. Second, the mother is concerned with establishing an emotional relationship with the baby, during which there is ideally a lessening of the mother’s own subjectivity for the purpose of being acutely in tune with the infant. The mother also develops a need to 62 create and maintain a protecting support network so that she can accomplish the tasks of motherhood. This support network ideally provides the mother with physical and practical help, as well as with emotional and psychological support. Stern asserts that support fiom females specifically, and also females that have had experiences with motherhood, is especially important. Finally, the fourth theme concerns the mother’s own representations of herself, as mentioned above, the mother must reorganize her identity to include representations of self-as—a mother, which are thought to be influenced by the mother’s representations of being cared for during childhood. Although there is a substantial amount of literature supporting Stern’s (1995) ideas, no empirical study to date has integrated this literature in a way that has been theorized and described by Stern. Furthermore, neither Stern’s theory nor most of the existing empirical studies have addressed processes related to the formation of mother- infant attachment in high-risk groups of women; instead, they have focused primarily on mother-infant attachment in well-educated, middle-class families. Therefore, this study will test many of Stern’s ideas, which are also consistent with attachment theory, within a high-risk group of women through a longitudinal research design (see Figure 1 for hypothesized model). Specific hypotheses for the current study were as follows: 1. Mothers’ representations of attachment during pregnancy, based on the recollection of experiences with caregivers, will be significantly related to mothers’ representations of their infants during pregnancy. More secure representations of attachment will be related to more secure or “balanced” representations of infants during pregnancy. 63 8.92 8835 .358 is. 0:89:00 .0633: 6:3 .98... .223 Ea:— .3 cctficomoaoa .8985 E25082 Ewan—LofioZ _ btaoow .5032 seemeom BESS). .5330; .5522 3 row we 53958.63. .8295 uncneu 68.8.. 2 :25 365.2: 963 as... Lon—.5 .592 .coE:uaz< he 22.25353— ?EBSZ coach: 239.00 .83. tome—m beam .023 38m 335%:— tonaam toga—am .335 833030 .332.— dozen—m £2.55 N_ 8 E25332 .5..:.-..u£e.2 he 9.29:5... .3358.— .Ea 36:3.— ..o .25.)— 25358»: atom 33:95 tenazw cozwueau redeem 78:22A— :aqzm 6285”,. ._ 25m: Mothers’ representations of attachment during pregnancy will be significantly related to the way in which mothers perceive themselves as caregivers during pregnancy. More secure representations of attachment will be related to more secure representations of the selfas a mother, characterized by a confident, but realistic view of the selfin the maternal role. Representations of the infant and representations of the selfas a mother during pregnancy will be complementary, but distinct, constructs and will be significantly related to each other. Mothers’ representations of the infant will be significantly related to mother- infant attachment security at 1 year postpartum. More secure, balanced representations of the infant during pregnancy will be related to greater infant security at 1 year. Thus, maternal representations of attachment will be indirectly related to mother-infant attachment at 1 year through its impact on representations of the infant. Representations of the self that emerge during pregnancy, as the women prepare for the. motherhood role, will be significantly related to mothers’ self-efficacy in the maternal role after the infant is born. More secure representations during pregnancy will be related to greater feelings of postnatal self-efficacy. Mothers’ self-efiicacy in the maternal role at 1 year postpartum will, in turn, be significantly related to infant attachment security. Greater feelings of efficacy will be related to greater attachment security. Thus, maternal representations of the selfas a mother and postnatal self-efficacy will be another pathway through 65 which mothers’ attachment will be indirectly related to infant attachment at 1 year. 7. Prenatal emotional support, practical support, and support with pregnancy-related issues fiom females specifically will be significantly related to postnatal, female support in the same three areas. That is, greater support during pregnancy will be related to greater support postpartum. 8. Postnatal support wilL in turn, be significantly related to maternal self-efficacy, with more support fiom females related to greater feelings of self-eflicacy. Thus, postnatal support will indirectly affect mother-infant attachment through its impact on maternal self-efficacy. 9. Several risk factors, including demographic risks, maternal depression, domestic violence, and child abuse and/or neglect, will be significantly related to mother- infant attachment, with more risk factors being related to less secure attachment. ExploratoLv Analyses In addition to testing Stern’s theory as depicted in Figure 1, exploratory analyses are proposed to assess the transmission of attachment over time using attachment categories from the three attachment measures (mothers’ representations of their own attachment, representations of the infant, and mother-infant attachment at 1 year). The degree of concordance between attachment categories is believed to be another important way of examining the continuity or discontinuity of attachment patterns between mother and infant. 66 CHAPTER 5: METHOD Participants Participants included 207 women, who were recruited and enrolled in conjunction with a larger study at Michigan State University. Women initially enrolled in the study during their last trimester of pregnancy (Time 1) and were interviewed again when their inEnts’ were approximately 2 months old (Time 2) and when their infants were approximately 12 to 15 months old (Time 3). At Time 2, one woman had a reported stillbirth and four women were unable to be located by research stafi‘. At Time 3, one woman who was missing at Time 2 was found and interviewed, another woman reported that her child had died and an additional 12 women were unable to be located or refused to participate. Thus, the retention rates were 98% and 92%, respectively. Women were recruited with flyers (see Appendix A) throughout the Clinton- Eaton-Ingham counties of Michigan at a number of agencies and clinics including: Obstetric/Gynecology clinics or other women’s health clinics (39%), flyers posted in the community at libraries, laundromats, stores, and other similar public places (27%), a number of social service programs such as FIA, W1C, Head Start, Jump Start, and Maternal Infant Outreach Program (26%), childbirth classes (5%), the county prosecutor’s ofice (2%), and a local domestic violence shelter (1%). General inclusion criteria included: 1) in the last trimester of pregnancy at the time of the initial interview, 2) 18 to 40 years of age, and 3) involvement in a romantic relationship for at least 6 weeks sometime during the pregnancy. Recruitment efi‘orts were made to include women who were ethnically and socioeconornically diverse. That is, specific recruitment sites were chosen to include women with a variety of backgrounds 67 and experiences. See Table 1 for demographic information on the entire sample. There were no significant demographic differences between participants who completed Time 2 and Time 3 interviews (9 = 190) and participants who did not complete follow-up interviews (a = 17), with the exception of Emily income; participants with missing data had significantly less income per month than participants with completed data (1 = 3.91 , p < .01 ). Procedures Women contacted the study office if they were interested in participating, at which time a research assistant conducted a brief screening to determine eligibility. The women were told the study was about women’s relationships with the important people in their life, including partners, family members, and children, and that if they participated in the study they would be asked about their thoughts and feelings about relationships and recent life events, including domestic violence. They were also told that they did not necessarily need to have experienced domestic violence in order to be eligible for the study. If the potential participant met criteria and agreed to participate, an appointment was made. Because the purpose of the larger study, with which this study was conducted, was to study the effects of domestic violence on women and infants, additional screening procedures and recruitment efl°orts were made to ensure that a reasonable number of subjects had in fact experienced domestic violence during the current pregnancy. After about halfthe sample had been recruited and interviewed (r_r = 96), the Conflict Tactics Scale (Straus, 1979) began to be administered over the telephone during the initial screening procedures in order to keep track of the number of battered and non-battered 68 Table 1 Demographic Characteristics of Study Participants (N = 207) Age of Mother M = 25.5 (SD = 5.0) Gender of Child" b Boy 104 (51%) Girl 99 (49%) Racial/Ethnic Group of Mother Caucasian 130 (63%) Afiican-American 53 (25%) Latina, Hispanic 10 (5%) Biracial 8 (4%) Native American 2 (1%) Asian American 1 (1%) Other 3 (1%) Racial/Ethnic Group of Child Caucasian 91 (44%) Afiican-American 47 (22%) Latina, Hispanic 4 (2%) Biracial 62 (30%) Asian American 1 (1%) Other 2 (1%) Marital Status Never Married 104 (50%) Married 83 (40%) Divorced 10 (5%) Separated 9 (4%) Widowed 1 (1%) Education Level of Mother High School 94 (45%) Some College 80 (39%) College Degree 16 (8%) Graduate Degree 11 (5%) Other 6 (3%) Income/Month M = $1806.29 (S_D = $1506.92) Socioeconomic Status M = 32.13 (SD = 11.17) ‘1 stillbirth b 3 unknown women. This screen was used to exclude women who had not experienced domestic violence during pregnancy once it was determined that there were enough non-battered women. After 137 participants had been recruited and interviewed, it was discovered that many of the “non-battered” women had actually experienced domestic violence in a prior 69 relationship. Thus, the telephone screen was then also used to enroll women who had never experienced domestic violence, in addition to those who experienced violence in the current pregnancy. Overall, 161 women who called the project office to participate were not included for the following reasons: did not meet battering experience criteria (a = 102), gave birth before the interview or could not be found again to schedule an interview (a = 41), did not meet relationship criteria (a = 9), planned to give baby up for adoption (p = 3), did not meet age criteria (a = 2), did not speak fluent English (a = 2), or decided not to participate after the phone screen (a = 2). There were no demographic differences between these excluded women and participants. _T__ime 1 I nmew Eight undergraduate students and five graduate students were trained to administer all questionnaires and a semi-structured interview (see description below) before Time 1 interviews were conducted. Research assistants attended a weekly training meeting for one semester and did two to five practice interviews under supervision until they reached 95% reliability for standard administration of measures. That is, interviewers were required to perform administrations with at least 95% accuracy. Throughout the period of data collection, research assistants continued to attend a weekly training meeting during which procedures were reviewed and interview problems were discussed. In addition. supervisors reviewed all completed interviews to ensure questionnaires and interviews were being administered correctly. During the Time 1 interviews, mothers were informed about anonymity and confidentiality and first completed an informed consent form (see Appendix B) that specified that her participation was voluntary and that she could withdraw fiom the study 70 without any negative consequences. Interviewers read all questionnaires aloud and marked down participant’s responses in order to control for variation in the level of literacy among participants. The semi-structured interview was audiotaped for later transcription and coding. Interviewers were blind to the battering status of the women, which was ensured by administering violence questionnaires last. Confidentiality was maintained by assigning all participants an identification number, which was placed on all data rather than participant names, and participant names were kept apart fiom the data. Time 1 interviews took place in the woman’s home or in the project oflice, whichever she preferred, and lasted approximately 3 hours. Participants were paid $50.00 and were given a list of community resources available for women and children. Time 2 Interview Participants were contacted by project staff approximately 1 week after the due date to confirm each infant’s date of birth. Women were contacted again approximately 6 to 8 weeks after the infant’s date of birth to set up the Time 2 interview, which nearly always took place between 2 and 3 months postpartum. During the first week of trying to contact women for this interview, research assistants made up to 10 calls to the participant’s home and up to four calls to her work. If the participant could not be reached by telephone (due to a disconnected phone, for example), a letter was sent to her home requesting that she call the office. If, by the second week, participants had not been reached, research assistants made up to two visits to her home. If the woman was not at home, a letter was left requesting that she contact the project ofice. If necessary, the following week, efforts were made to contact women through ‘recontact’ people, whose names, addresses, and telephone numbers were provided by participants during 71 their first interview. Recontact people were both called (up to five times) and were contacted directly through several visits to their homes. If recontact people were not at home, letters were left asking them to forward the information to participants. Eight research assistants were trained to administer the Time 2 interview by conducting several practice interviews with each other, observing a supervisor conduct a real interview, and doing their first interview with a participant under supervision. During data collection, interviewers attended a weekly meeting to discuss difliculties that arose during interviews, as well as strategies to reach women who were “missing.” Time 2 interviews were primarily done over the telephone (less than 10 participants did the interview in person because they had no phone) and lasted about 30 to 40 minutes. Information about confidentiality was read aloud to participants before the interview began (see Appendix C). All questionnaires were read aloud, and interviewers wrote down women’s responses. Women were given a baby gift worth $5.00 following the completion of the interview. Trac ' Partici ants Over Time After the Time 2 interview, women were followed through the infants’ first year of life every 90 days in order to stay in touch with women and minimize attrition. Thus, they were contacted at about 5 months, 8 months, and 11 to 12 months postpartum. At each follow-up point, a letter and information sheet (asking for the participant’s current address and phone numbers) were sent to the participant (see Appendix D), along with a self-addressed stamped envelope. In addition to returned information sheets, the post office sent a postcard to the project oflice with current addresses for those participants who had moved and registered a new address. Participants were called directly if they 72 did not return the information sheet or if the post office did not send new inforrmtion within 3 weeks. If participants were unable to be reached directly at that point, efforts were made to contact them through their recontact people. If all other tracking methods failed, research assistants went to the participants’ and recontact people’s homes. Time 3 Interview When participants’ infants were approximately 11 to 12 months old, women were contacted using the same tracking procedures described above, to see if they were willing to participate in a follow-up interview with their infants. Although efforts were made to complete all Time 3 interviews within two weeks before or after the infant’s date of birth, this was not always possible due to problems finding and scheduling participants within that time fiame. Thus, 54% of the interviews were completed when infants were 11 to 12 months old, 39% were completed when infants were 13 to 14 months old, 5% were completed when infants were 15 to 16 months old, and 2% were completed when infants were 17 to 18 months Old. A total of 13 undergraduate students and three graduate students were trained to conduct the mother and infant assessments at Time 3. Training consisted of weekly meetings and outside activities over a 7-month period. Training activities included: 1) learning about infant development, assessment, and mother-infant attachment through readings and discussion, 2) as many practice interviews as was necessary to achieve an accuracy rate of at least 85% for administration of all measures, including interviews and videotaped procedures, and 3) observation of supervisors’ conducting real interviews. In addition, approximately 20% of the interviews were observed by supervisors in order to control for drift in standard administration. 73 The Time 3 interview took place in the project office because video equipment was required for some laboratory procedures. Mothers first completed an informed consent form for herself and her infant (see Appendix E). Mothers and infants then participated in a standardized laboratory procedure together (described below) to assess the quality of their attachment. Afterwards, one research assistant met with the mother to administer interviews and questionnaires, while another research assistant administered a few tests to the infant in a separate room (for the purposes of the larger study). The entire Time 3 interview lasted about 3 to 4 hours. Women were paid $75.00 and given a baby gift worth approximately $8.00 after completion of the interview. Measures Maternal Representations of Attachment (Time 1 ) Mothers’ representations of attachment were assessed by the Perceptions of Adult Attachment Questionnaire (PAAQ, formerly called the Inventory of Adult Attachment; Lichtenstein & Cassidy, 1991), a 60-item self report that asks about experiences during childhood and the participant’s perceptions of her mother as a caregiver, as well as the participant’s relationship with her mother (see Appendix F). All items are rated on a 1 to 5 Likert scale ranging fi‘om Strongly DiagLe' e to Strongly Aggee. Items form eight subscales: 1) Rejection by mother (11 items), 2) Loved by mother (6 items), 3) Role- Reversal with mother (10 items), 4) Anger towards mother (5 items), 5) Derogation of attachment experiences (4 items), 6) Forgiving of childhood problems (7 items), 7) feelings of Vulnerability about relationship with mother (5 items), and 8) No Memory of childhood experiences (4 items). Scales 1-5 and 8 were designed to be equivalent to six 74 scales from the Adult Attachment Interview. Higher scores on each subscale indicate higher levels of the construct being measured, e.g., more rejection, more love, etc... Lichtenstein and Cassidy (1991) indicated that the PAAQ scales are fairly internally consistent; alpha coefiicients ranged fiom .62 (Derogation) to .90 (No Memory) in a sample of 247 college students and fiom .51 (Derogation) to .94 (No Memory) in a sample of 123 mothers. Test-retest reliabilities ranged from .64 to .86 in the college sample and were not reported for the mother sample. The authors compared PAAQ responses to AAI responses in the sample of mothers and found that equivalent scales were highly correlated (p < .01) with each other, with the exception of the Role- Reversal and Derogation scales. Thus, since the AAI is currently the “gold standar ” for the measurement of adults’ representations of attachment, it appears that most of the PAAQ scales have good comment validity. Based on attachment theory and that underlying the AAI classifications, respondents can presumably be classified into Autonomous, Dismissive, and Preoccupied attachment groups based on their scores on the eight subscales. Theoretically, autonomous adults would report more experiences of love and feelings of forgiveness, dismissive adults would report more rejection, derogation, and a lack of memory of childhood experiences, and preoccupied adults would report role-reversal, angry feelings, and feelings of vulnerability (Hesse, 1999). However, a confirmatory factor analysis on the current sample revealed that six of the eight subscales loaded onto a more general attachment factor that measures a continuum of security rather than qualitatively distinct attachment styles (see the Results chapter for more details). Therefore, only those six subscales (Rejection, Love, Anger, Derogation, Vulnerability, and No Memory) were used 75 to measure matemal representations of attachment. Coefficient alphas for the current sample were: Rejection = .91 , Love = .92, Anger = .79, Derogation = .49, Vulnerability = .65, No Memory = .93. Finally, although the AAI is the gold standard for measuring maternal representations of attachment, the PAAQ was chosen in this study due to limited time and resources, as well as the inclusion of another labor-intensive clinical interview (see below). Despite the fact that a number of researchers have challenged the use of questionnaires to measure attachment representations, noting that it is difiicult to assess defensive and narrative processes from questionnaire data (e.g., Crowell & Treboux, 1995), there is reason to believe that questionnaire data can be useful. For example, many adults can and do provide accurate information on their experiences, and conscious and unconscious processes often Operate in a similar way to achieve the same goal (see Crowell, Fraley, & Shaver, 1999 for a review of these issues). Prenatal Rgpresentations of Infant and of Self-As-Mother (Tm The Working Model of the Child Interview (WMCI; Zeanah et al., 1994) was used to measure matemal representations of the infant and of the self-as-mother during pregnancy. This instrument is a 1 to 1 1/2 hour structured interview that inquires about a participant’s perceptions and subjective experiences of her infant and relationship with her infant. Interviews were audiorecorded, transcribed, and rated by trained coders along 13 5-point Likert scales that assess qualitative (e.g., coherence of interview), content (e.g., infant difficulty), and affective features (i.e., anger, joy) of maternal representations. Higher scores indicate higher levels of the construct being measured. 76 Coders then assign an overall classification to the narratives provided by the mothers based on the scale profiles (with an emphasis on the qualitative scales). Balanced narratives include both positive and negative characteristics of the infirm and relationship with the infant. They convey value for the infant’s individuality and appreciation for the infant’s subjective experience. The caregiver’s perceptions are open to change and are at least moderately rich in detail about the infant and the caregiving experiences. Disengaged representations are characterized by emotional distance or indifference towards the infant. Caregivers are unable to recognize the inEnt’s individuality, and if it is recognized, it is not valued. Details about the infant or parenting experience lack richness, are not flexible or open to new experience, and are emotionally unintegrated. Finally, Distorted representations reflect general inconsistencies. Caregivers may be preoccupied or overwhelmed by the infant or may have unrealistic expectations about the infant. Unlike disengaged parents, distorted mothers do not deny their impact on their infants. However, they often do not recognize how their behaviors may be detrimental to the infants. Descriptions of infants and the relationship with the infant may be incoherent or contradictory. Much feeling is expressed toward the infant, but these emotions lack a sense of modulation or meaning. Several studies have demonstrated predictive and concurrent validity for the WMCI by reporting highly significant relationships between WMCI classifications and infant attachment classifications assessed by the Strange Situation in the expected direction: Balanced-Secure, Disengaged-Avoidant, and Distorted-Ambivalent (Benoit et al., 1997; Zeanah et al., 1994). Another study provided more evidence for validity by showing a significant relationship between the disengaged and distorted representations 77 and high-risk/disordered infant status (Benoit, Zeanah, Parker, Nicholson, & Coolbear, 1997). Interrater agreement has ranged fiom .57 to .76 (Cohen’s kappa) for different classifications in previous studies, and Benoit et al. (1997) reported a high degree of test- retest stability for the Secure and Distorted classifications (concordance over 1 year’s time was 89% and 85% respectively). As of yet, there is no evidence of stability for the Disengaged classification. In the present study, questions were changed to future tense to inquire about mothers’ representations during pregnancy (see Appendix G). Benoit et al. (1997) has demonstrated that this is a valid technique for assessing prenatal representations. Those subscales that measured qualitative features (with some content features) of maternal representations were used to assess the mothers’ representations of their infants, since these are believed to be most theoretically important. These included: 1) Richness of Perceptions, which measures the poverty or richness of representations of the infant and the degree to which the mother “knows” the infant, 2) Openness to Change, which measures the flexibility of the mother’s representation to accommodate new information about the infant (compared to rigidity), 3) Coherence, which measures the overall organization of the mother’s narrative about the infant and her relationship with the infant and the logical flow of her responses, 4) Caregiving Sensitivity, which assesses the degree to which the mother recognizes and responds adequately to the infant’s own needs and experiences including a respect for the infant as a separate but dependent individual, and 5) Acceptance, which measures the degree to which the mother is open and accepting of responsibilities involved with adequate caretaking. 78 The author and another graduate student were trained to code scales and classify representations according to the coding system developed by Zeanah et al. (1994), with the assistance of another research group that has been trained by Zeanah. Adequate inter- rater reliability was established using weighted-kappa (or corrected-kappa) (Cohen, 1968; Fleiss, Cohen, & Everitt, 1969) for subscales and both percent agreement and Cohen’s kappa for classifications. These were calculated based on 26 interviews that were coded by both trained coders (13% of the sample). Weighted kappas for the subscales used in this study were as follows: Richness = .68, Openness = .51, Coherence = .62, Sensitivity = .69, Acceptance = .77. Percent agreement for overall classification was 96%, with a kappa of .94 (p < .001). In addition to the five scales assessing representations of the infant, mothers’ responses on the WMCI were coded for representations of self-as-mother. This code was adapted fiom the Confidence and Competence scale from Slade et aL’s (1994) Pregnancy Interview Coding System. This subscale assesses the mother’s representations of her own competence and self-efficacy in the maternal role and her expectations of herselfas a mother along a 5-point Likert scale. Unlike the other qualitative scales developed by Zeanah et a1. (1994), the middle point on this scale represents the most “balan ” or “secure” representation of self-as-mother; mothers who score low on this scale lack confidence in themselves, while mothers high on this scale are overly and unrealistically self-confident (which is believed to be a defensive response to underlying ineflicacy). Thus, a balanced mother would recognize her strengths and limitations and acknowledge the challenges of motherhood, but would overall feel that she is able to make her baby feel happy, safe, and secure. For analyses, codes of 2 and 4 were collapsed and codes of 79 1 and 5 were collapsed so that the highest possible code was a 3, which represented the most balanced representations of self. Weighted kappa for this subscale was .63. Maternal Self-Efficacy (Time 3) The Maternal Efficacy Questionnaire (MEQ; Teti & Gelfand, 1991) was used to measure maternal self-efficacy, or the mother’s perceptions about the degree to which she is able to perform effectively as a parent, when the infant was approximately 12 months old. This instrument is made up of 10 items, rated on a 4-point Likert scale, that tap feelings of efficacy related to a number of specific domains of parenting such as soothing the infant (see Appendix H). A total efficacy score is obtained by summing all items, and higher scores indicate greater self-efficacy. Teti and Gelfand (1991) originally administered the scale to 86 mothers, about halfof whom were clinically depressed, with infants between the ages of 3 and 13 months. They reported a coeflicient alpha of .86, indicating good internal reliability. Furthermore, they demonstrated good concurrent validity by reporting a high correlation between the MEQ and the Parenting Stress Index Sense of Competence Scale (1 = -.75). Coeflicient alpha in the current study was .65. Mother-Infant Attachment (Time 3) The Strange Situation (SS; Ainsworth et al., 1978) is a well-known laboratory procedure that was used to measure mother-infant attachment (see Appendix I). The procedure consists of eight episodes, each lasting about 3 minutes with the exception of the first episode, which lasts about 30 seconds. The entire procedure takes approximately 21 minutes and is videotaped and coded at a later time. Infant behaviors during the SS are coded on four 7-point scales including proximity seeking, contact maintaining, avoidance, and resistance according to a coding 80 system developed by Ainsworth et al. (1978) and one 9-point scale for disorganization according to a coding system developed by Main and Solomon (1990). Infant behaviors during reunion episodes are particularly important. Based on patterns of infant behavior toward the caregiver, the quality of mother-infant attachment is also determined, and infants are classified into one of four general categories: Secure (Type B), Insecure- Avoidant (Type A), Insecure-Ambivalent (Type C), and Disorganized/Disoriented (Type D). Infants can be further classified within the three original categories; there are four types of Secure (Bl — B4), two types ofInsecure-Avoidant (A1 and A2), and two types of Insecure-Ambivalent attachment (C1 and C2). See Table 2 for a description of each subgroup. Reliability and validity of this instrument have been reported by numerous studies (i.e., Ainsworth et al., 1978; see Solomon & George, 1999 for a review). OveralL attachment classifications have been shown to be highly stable, with somewhat less stability in disadvantaged samples. Interrater reliability is very high (about 85% to 95%) for researchers trained by Ainsworth and her students. In addition, repeated studies have found a significant relationship between theoretically related variables and inEnt attachment such as matemal sensitivity and responsiveness, as well as between attachment and later expected outcomes such as mental health and social competence. In the present study, SS videotapes were sent away and coded by trained, reliable coders at the University of Washington (under the direction of Dr. Susan Spieker, who has extensive experience coding SS tapes). Coders achieved a 90% agreement rate on overall classifications, with a kappa of .84 (p < .001), based on double-coding 11% of the sample. SS categories were then converted by the current investigator into a continuous 81 Table 2 Descriptions of Original Attachment Classifications by Subgpoup Classification Description Type B: Secure B3 B1 32 B4 Type A: Avoidant A1 A2 Type_ C: Ambivalent C1 C2 Most secure group. Strong proximity-seeking and contact maintaining. Little or no Sign of avoiding or resisting caregiver. Predominantly secure behaviors, with some evidence of avoidance. May greet mother, but no strong proximity- seeking Little or no resistance. Predominantly secure behaviors, with some evidence of avoidance. More proximity-seeking than B] but not as much as B3. Little or no resistance. Predominantly secure behaviors, with some evidence of preoccupation with caregiver and may show some resistance. Little or no avoidance. Most avoidant group, showing active avoidance of caregiver. Little or no proximity-seeking or contact- maintaining. Predominantly avoidant behaviors, but with some tendency to greet caregiver or seek proximity. Strong resistance and ambivalent behavior towards caregiver, combined with extreme distress and unmistakable angry quality. Strong resistance and ambivalent behavior towards caregiver, combined with a high degree of passivity. Not as angry as C]. scale assessing the degree of felt security according to the conversion system suggested by Bretherton and colleagues (Bretherton et al., 1989; Bretherton et al. 1990): B3 = 5; Bl,BZ,B4=4;A2,C1 =3;A1,C2=2;D= 1. Thissystemisbasedonthefindingsof 82 Ainsworth et al. (1978), which showed that mothers of B3 infants are most sensitive, followed by mothers of B1, B2, and B4 infants, and so on, and is also consistent with the security continuum suggested by Cummings (1990). There are a number of reasons why it is useful to assess attachment security along a continuum (see Cummings, 1990, for a review). First, not all mother-infant attachments fit neatly into “prototype” categories. Even when attachments are classifiable, there may be significant differences in felt security within the broad attachment classifications (within Type B, for example), which are generally used by researchers due to limited sample sizes. In addition, when classifications appear to be on the borderline between two groups, the use of a security continuum can reduce the possibility of measurement error. Cummings also argues that assessment along a continuum is conceptually justified because the infant’s felt security comes from the attachment figure and is central to the function of attachment. Maternal Social Suppprt (Time 1 and Time 3) The Norbeck Social Support Questionnaire (N SSQ; Norbeck, Lindsey, & Carrieri, 1981) was administered to women during their pregnancy and again at 12 months after birth to measure mothers’ level of social support (see Appendix J). The NSSQ assesses multiple dimensions of social support including the quality of Emotional Support, Practical Support (or Aid), the size of the social support network (the number of supporters available to the participant), the duration of support, and the fi'equency of contact with supporters. Three items, which specifically asked about support related to pregnancy/preparation for motherhood (during pregnancy) and related to caregiving (at 83 12 months), were developed for this study and added to the original 10 items (see Appendix K for postnatal additional items). Norbeck et al. (1981) originally tested internal consistency through item- intercorrelations based on a sample of college students. They reported correlations between .72 to .98, with items on the same subscale correlating more highly. Test-retest reliabilities ranged fiom .85 to .90 over a one-week period and .58 to .78 over a seven month period (Norbeck, Lindsey, & Carrieri, 1982). Norbeck et al. (1981, 1982) reported good concurrent validity by showing significant correlations between NS SQ scales and other well-known social support scales. In addition, Norbeck et al. (1982) reported evidence of construct validity by demonstrating that practical support served as a buffer between life stress and negative mood, as would be expected by theory. All items are rated on a 0 to 4 scale (ranging fiom Not at all to A gpeat deal). The authors suggest summing the amount of support received by each supporter on each item and then summing item totals for the Emotional Support and Practical Support scales, with the assumption that participants with larger networks receive a greater amount of social support (Norbeck, 1995). However, average satisfaction can also be calculated by dividing summed totals by the number of supporters and may be a better indicator of the quality of support. In the present study, the relationship of each supporter to the participant, as well as each supporter’s gender, was obtained so that support fiom females specifically could be measured. Thus, average satisfaction from female supporters only was calculated for Emotional Support, Practical (Aid) Support, and Caregiving Support (support related to pregnancy and motherhood), and these scores were used to assess the quality of prenatal and postnatal social support. 84 Risk Factors (Time 1. Tim_e 2. and Time_3_) Several risk factors were assessed at all three interviews in order to examine the number and types of risk factors participants had experienced, as well as their impact on mother-infant attachment. Risk factors included demographic risks (e. g., poverty), maternal depression, domestic violence experiences, and child abuse and neglect. In order to assess demographic risks, a demographic questionnaire (see Appendix L) was administered during pregnancy to obtain basic information such as women’s ethnicity, education and occupation, family income, and Emily composition. Socioeconomic status (SES) was computed according to Hollingshead Index (1975). SES was calculated based on the participant only if she was unmarried Md not living with a partner, while SES was calculated based on both participant and partner if the participant was either married or living with a partner. A demographic risk scale was created from several demographic characteristics believed to put women and infants at greater risk for problems. These characteristics included: a) income below the poverty level (based on income and family size as defined by the US. Census bureau, 2001), b) socioeconomic status in the lowest quartile of the sample, and c) single parenthood, defined as unmarried or not living with a partner. Participants received one “point” for each characteristic, thus, scores ranged fi'om 0 to 3. For example, if a woman’s income was below the poverty level an_d_ she was a single parent, she would receive a score of 2. The Beck Depression Inventory (BDI; Beck, Ward, Mendelson, Mock, & Erbaugh, 1961), a 21-item self-report, was used to assess the severity of maternal depression at several time points. The instrument covers a variety of symptoms of depression such as depressed mood, pessimism, sleep disturbances, and changes in 85 appetite (see Appendix M). Each item consists of four statements that are ranked fiom neutral to severe, and values fiom 0 to 3 are assigned to each statement. For example, question 1 consists of the statements “I do not feel sad,” “I feel sad,” “I am sad all the time and I can’t snap out of it,” and “I am so sad or unhappy that I can’t stand it.” The total depression score is obtained by summing the answers of all 21 items. Possrble scores range fiom 0-63, with scores of 0-9 indicating no depression, 10-15 indicating mild depression, 16-23 indicating moderate depression, and 24-63 indicating severe depression. The BDI was originally designed for use in psychiatric populations. Beck et al. (1961) reported that split-half reliability estimates yielded a coefficient of .93 after a Spearman-Brown correction, indicating high internal consistency. They also examined the validity by comparing BDI scores to psychiatric ratings of depression and found a correlation coeflicient of .67, which was highly significant. A later study (Bumberry, Oliver, & McClure, 1978) demonstrated the validity of the instrument in a university population. Using psychiatric ratings as the criterion, Bumberry et al. found a correlation coeficient of .77 between the BDI and interview ratings. In the present study, depression was assessed at Time 1, Time 2, and Time 3. Coeficient alphas were .85, .89, and .85, respectively. Women who obtained a score of 16 or higher (moderately to severely depressed) were considered “at-risk,” and a cumulative depression risk score was calculated, ranging from O to 3. Thus, if a wormn scored 16 or higher at Time 2 only, she received a score of 1, while a woman with scores of 16 or higher at two points in time received a score of 2. 86 The Severity of Violence Against Women Scales (SVAWS; Marshall, 1992) was used to measure the type and severity of domestic violence, defined here as male-to— female violence in adult partners, experienced by mothers at multiple time periods. This self-report contains 46 items which make up nine dimensions of violence ranging from threats of mild violence to severe physical and sexual violence. In the original sample of community women, coefficient alphas ranged fiom .89 to .96, indicating high internal consistency within dimensions. In the present study, women filled out the scale for several time periods including: a) during her pregnancy with the infant, b) the year before pregnancy, c) for a previous partner (all ofthese assessed at Time 1, see Appendix N), and d) during the firstyearof the infant’s life (assessed at Time 3, see Appendix 0). Coeficient alphas for each time period were: .95, .97, .98, and .95. Because it is believed that experiencing domestic violence in multiple relationships and/or for longer periods of time is more harmful for women and child witnesses than single, brief episodes of abuse, the experience of physical and/or sexual abuse within each time period was considered as a separate risk factor in order to calculate a cumulative domestic violence risk score. For example, a woman who reported abuse during pregnancy and during the infant’s first year of life received a score of 2. Abuse is defined here as threats of moderate or severe violence, mild to serious violence, or sexual violence (as indicated by any item on the scale between 9 and 46). Scores ranged fi'om 0 (no abuse at any time periods) to 4 (abuse at all four time periods). Physical abuse and/or neglect of the participating inEnt were measured in two ways at Time 3. First, mothers completed the Parent Behavior Checklist (PBC; Fox, 87 1994), a 100-item questionnaire on parenting behaviors of infants and toddlers. Each item is rated on a 4-point Likert scale ranging fi'om _l_\l_e_veg to _Al_yv_ay§. The scale comprises three subscales, however, only the Discipline subscale was used in the present study. This subscale includes 30 items and assesses the amount of physical punishment exerted by the parent (see Appendix P). Higher scores indicate better discipline techniques and less physical punishment. Fox (1994) reported good reliability and validity for the PBC; coeflicient alpha for the Discipline subscale was .91, test-retest reliability was .87, all items had a loading of at least .30 for the Discipline factor, and the scale was highly correlated with previously-developed parenting questionnaires. In the present study, coeficient alpha for the Discipline subscale was .85. A second way that child abuse/neglect was assessed in the current study was through a 30-item child neglect questionnaire developed for the larger study (Bogat & Levendosky, 1999) with which the present study was being conducted (see Appendix Q). Each item is rated on the same Likert scale as the PBC, and a total neglect score is obtained by summing all items, with higher scores indicating more neglect. The coeflicient alpha for this scale was .66. For purposes of the present study, a child abuse risk score was calculated using both measures of abuse/neglect. Participants received one “point” if their scores on the PBC Discipline scale were among the lowest quartile of the sample and one point if their scores on the Neglect scale were among the top quartile of the sample. For example, if a woman’s score on the Neglect scale only was among the highest quartile in the sample, she would receive a score of 1. Thus, higher scores indicated more child abuse risk. 88 CHAPTER 6: RESULTS Missmg' Data Due to attrition over time and a small number of data collection errors, several pieces of data were missing at all time periods. At Time 1, social support and WMCI data were missing for one participant because the interview was interrupted and the woman was never located to finish the interview (r_r = 206). WMCI data were also missing for four additional participants due to recording errors (a = 202). At Time 2, maternal depression was missing on five participants; researchers could not locate four women and one additional woman had a reported stillbirth (r_r = 202). All Time 3 variables were missing on 17 participants; one woman had a stillbirth, one woman’s child died about six months after birth, nine women were unable to be located by researchers, and six women refused to do the interview (a = 190). Time 3 questionnaires about the child (PBC, MEQ) were missing on two additional women due to having no contact with their babies since shortly after birth (and thus, they could not fill out the measures in a valid manner) (p = 188). Finally, there were 10 additional missing Strange Situation videotapes; one was accidentally not recorded on tape, one woman refused to do the taped portions of the interview, one woman lost custody of her baby and could not bring the baby into the lab (although she filled out questionnaires about the child), and seven women lived out of state and were only able to complete questionnaires through the mail (p = 178). In order to maximize statistical power, all missing data were estimated using the imputation method known as the “hot-deck” method from the Lisrel 8.5/Prelis 2.0 software (Jdreskog & SOrbom, 2001) after all recodings and data transformations. This 89 method of estimation substitutes real values for missing values. Real values are obtained fiom another participant’s responses that are most closely matched to the participant with missing data on a set of specified matching variables. This procedure has several advantages: 1) the values that are substituted into missing data points are within the actual range of possible scores on that particular measure, and 2) the results of the imputation are not affected by the order of cases in the database. In the current study, missing data points were estimated based on the following set of matching variables in the database: income, Time 1 maternal depression, Time 1 domestic violence scores (all three time periods measured at Time 1), and Time 1 PAAQ subscales (all six). Estimation was successful, evidenced by the very similar descriptive data for measures both before and after estimation (see Table 3). Thus, measurement and structural model testing was based on data from 207 participants. Data Reduction See Table 3 for descriptive data on all measures in the hypothesized model. As the table shows, all measures had adequate variance with the possible exception of Time 3 self-emcacy, which was heavily skewed towards the positive end of the scale. All variables had satisfactory distributions as evidenced by skewness and kurtosis values in the appropriate ranges (: 2 and i 4), with the exception of Time 1 and Time 3 Emotional Support. The latter support variables had elevated kurtosis scores above 4. Because the cumulative risk variables were created fi'om multiple measures and in order to help illustrate the numbers and specific types of risk factors experienced by participants over time, Tables 4 to 7 are provided. See Appendix R for the correlation matrix of model variables. 90 8a a: 25 2-0 2a was 5&5 3235 .2 2.: :3. no we 8.: man eaaam .8228: .2 NS 8.: 2a 2a EN 2.: e895 3895 .2 ladle am alaolm l: calm 8a 2.2 2.2 as: an 2.: sea 35 .2 “Owen a. 2a 2 E 8. a3 sec: 828.8 as: .2 8.: 2a a. m: 8: 2.2.. nonsense. .: a. :3 a: E 3. cam 22:83 .2 a. EX 2 a: 3. and 8:228 .a 2. 2a 3 2 2. 2a 2:20 a 8580 .w a... 32.. E 3 8. mam 22385 no anion: s ”i523 $2. 2.2 a? 84 Se 2.2 been: oz 6 2a 2.2 Ba new 23 8.2 0:855 .m SN :3 84 2-2. New .3 3225 .4 Ra. :..: Ra Ra Re :1 _ 35.. .m an 2.8 2-0 one 22 :18 ”>3 .N a.” 2.8 2-: 2-: a.” 2.8 :88? ._ Hallie 33 0.8.33 38 220$ 9222230 .2328 323533 2323 Om 5.62 amend 03239“ mocoom we amend Gm 522 c.5982 flow u E .252 wofimofiommm 05 5 mogmmlolvz .8.“ flea 339.880 m 033. 91 3.— me. 5.— mm. 8.. 5N“... mm; wofi. Nm.m Nm. mm.— mm. co.— 3.x. 36 him _ NTo We 26 NT: mé oTO voN ow; 3N an 525% 285822 «N ”notwamm o 5.5m 2. 2830223.... EEO .2 an: 853$ 028.8 .mm mm. commmoaoo 3:532 Am 8: a2 02.28388 .8 3 m3 2225 3:35 .2 26 e895 388.: .2 :2 eaaam 5285 .2 23% APEoov m 2an 92 Table 4 Breakdown of Participants Emriencmg’ Demographic Risks Number of Risk Factors Frequency 0 88 (42%) 1 53 (26%) 2 44 (21%) 3 22 (11%) Table 5 Breakdown of Participag' ts Experiencgg' Depression Number of Times Frequency 0 154 (74%) 1 34 (17%) 2 15 (7%) 3 4 (2%) Table 6 Breakdown of Particgp' ants Experiencmg' Domestic Violence Number of Times Frequency 0 52 (25%) 1 56 (27%) 2 46 (22%) 3 31 (15%) 4 22 (11%) Table 7 Breakdown of Particm’ ants with Child Abuse/Neglect Ris_k Number of Risk Factors Frequency 0 121 (58%) 1 61 (30%) 25 (12%) 93 Measurement Models Before testing the overall model shown in Figure l, confirmatory factor analyses (CFA) were conducted for each latent construct with more than one indicator, in order to determine the adequacy of the measurement models. In general, all CF A models were specified so that the residual variances of the factors and indicators were estimated, assuming that both factors and indicators had some measurement error. Residual covariances were freed as needed, when this led to a significantly improved model fit. All measurement model estimation was done using the Unweighted Least Squares (ULS) method, and models were specified on the y-side. A good fit was determined by: a non- significant chi-square value and/or a chi-square value that was less than two times the degrees of fi'eedom, a Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA) value of less than .05, and a Goodness of Fit Index (GFI) and Adjusted Goodness of Fit Index (AGFI) between .90 and 1.00 (Hoyle & Panter, 1995; Schumacker & Lormx, 1996; Ullman, 1996). Lisrel 8.5 was used for all measurement models. The first CF A was conducted for the construct Maternal Representations of Attachment. This factor had six indicators, which were the six subscales of the PAAQ. In order to obtain a good model fit, residual covariances were freed for the following indicators: Derogation and No Memory, Rejection and Love, Anger and Vulnerability, Rejection and No Memory, and Rejection and Anger. Subsequently, the model had an excellent fit according to several goodness-of-fit indices. The _X_2 value was 4.25, df = 4, with p = .37. The RMSEA was .02, GFI = 1.00, and AGFI = 1.00. All factor loadings were significant. See Appendix S. 94 The second CFA examined the construct Prenatal Representation of the Infant. This factor had five indicators, which included the five qualitative and content subscales of the WMCI. The model fit was excellent. Chi-square = 6.64, df= 5, p = .25, RMSEA = .04, GFI = 1.00, and AGFI= 1.00. No residual covariances needed to be estimated. All factor loadings were significant. See Appendix T. The third and fourth CF A analyses examined the Prenatal Social Support and Postnatal Social Support constructs. Both factors had three indicators each, which included Female Emotional Support, Female Practical Support, and Female Caregiving support as measured by the Time 1 and Time 3 NSSQ. Both measurement models were initially saturated, so in order to obtain more parsimonious models, residual variances for the first and third indicators were set equal. The resulting model for prenatal support showed a very good fit: 3} = 1.99, df= 1, 12 = .16, RMSEA = .07, GFI = 1.00, and AGFI = 1.00. All factor loadings were significant. See Appendix U. The model for postnatal support was not as strong with only some indices showing a good fit: X2 = 10.72, df = 1, p < .05, RMSEA = .22, GFI = 1.00, and AGFI = .98. All factor loadings were significant. See Appendix V. Finally, a CFA was conducted for the Risk Factors construct. This factor initially had four indicators including the demographic risk scale, maternal depression risk scale, domestic violence risk scale, and the child abuse/neglect risk scale. The model had an excellent fit according to several indices: 32 = 3.90, df= 2, p = .14, RMSEA = .07, GFI = 1.00, and AGFI = .98. No residual covariances needed to be freed. All factor loadings were significant. See Appendix W. 95 After the full model was tested and modified (see full description below), the risk construct was changed to include only Time 1 risk factors. Thus, the demographic risk scale was unchanged, the maternal depression scale was changed to a dichotomous variable for presence/absence of depression at Time 1, the domestic violence scale was changed to a 0 to 3 scale by eliminating Time 3 domestic violence (thus, only including the three time periods of: pregnancy, year before pregnancy, and previous partner), and the child abuse/neglect scale was eliminated completely. The means and standard deviations for the new depression and violence scales were M = .20, S_D = .40 and M = 1.29, SD = 1.05, respectively. Skewness and Kurtosis values were within the appropriate ranges. The new frequencies for these risk variables are shown in Table 8. A CFA was then conducted on this new Risk Factors construct, which included three indicators of Time 1 risks. The model was initially saturated, so the residual variances for the first and second indicators were set equal in order to obtain a more parsimonious model. Subsequently, the model fit was good: X2 = 4.68, df= 2, p = .09, RMSEA = .08, GFI = .98, and AGFI = .95. No residual covariances needed to be freed. Factor loadings were significant except for ‘bdirisk’, which was almost significant (1 = 1.38). See Appendix X. Table 8 . Breakdown of Participants Experiencing Depression and Domestic Violence at Time 1 Number of Times Frequency Depression: 0 166 (80%) 1 41 (20%) Domestic Violence: 0 58 (28%) 1 66 (32%) 2 49 (24%) 3 34 (16%) 96 Full Structural and Measurement Model Model testing was guided by the theoretically-derived model shown in Figure 1. The model included 8 latent variables and 24 indicators, all of which were defined as endogenous variables. Model testing was based on the covariance matrix of the indicators, using Lisre18.5 (Jereskog & Sbrbom, 2001). The matrix of factor loadings was specified as fill], indicating an asymmetric matrix and allowing for reciprocal relationships between variables. The theta-epsilon matrix (indicator residuals) and the psi matrix (factor residuals) were specified as symmetrical and fixed. Residual variances of the factors and indicators were estimated, assuming that both factors and indicators had some measurement error. Criteria for a good model fit were the same as those used to evaluate the measurement models. The method of estimation used was Maximum Likelihood (ML), which is the most fi'equently used and recommended method of estimation for SEM analyses (Ullman, 1996). This method maximizes the likelihood for parameter estimates given the data and performs well under less than optimal conditions (Hoyle & Panter, 1995). Results initially revealed that the data poorly fit the model. The £2 value was 563.72, df = 243, p < .05. In addition, the RMSEA was .11. Although the GFI = .92 and the AGFI = .90, other fit indices such as the Non-Normed Fit Index and the Comparative Fit Index had negative values. Overall, these indices indicated significant problems with the model. The modification indices provided by the program were used to guide modifications to the model, along with a determination about whether or not a modification made theoretical sense. One of the first modifications to the model was changing the two-way arrow between prenatal representations of the infant and of the self 97 as mother to a one-way arrow predicting representations of the selffrpp; representations of the infant. The next major modification was to eliminate the estimated parameter between the Risk Factors construct and the Mother-Infant Attachment construct. Instead, the data suggested that Risk Factors more strongly predicted Prenatal Representations of the Infant. However, this posed conceptual problems because the predictor, Risk Factors, included variables measured at Time 3, while Representations of the Infant was measured at Time 1. Temporally, it would be impossible for a Time 3 variable to predict a Time 1 variable. Therefore, the Risk Factors construct was changed to only include Time 1 variables (see description of the new measurement model above). Subsequently, the pathway fi'om Risk Factors to Prenatal Representations of the Infant was fieed and estimated. These changes substantially improved the model (see Appendix Y for the revised correlation matrix of variables with the new risk variables, upon which the final model was based). In order to obtain a good model fit according to the criteria defined above, several other modifications were made. Because there were three factors with only one indicator, several residual values were set to a fixed value of 1 (for the observed variables postnatal self eflicacy and infant attachment security). In addition, covariances between the following pairs of latent constructs were freed: l) Maternal Representations of Attachment and Prenatal Social Support, 2) Postnatal Social Support and Mother-Infant Attachment, and 3) Prenatal Representations of Infiint and Mother-Infant Attachment. Finally, a number of residual covariances between pairs of observed variables were freed and estimated’. Although more modifications were suggested by the program to further improve the model fit, no modifications were made after a good fit was established. 98 The final model consisted of eight latent factors and 23 indicators (See Figure 2). The method of estimation was Maximum Likelihood and model testing was based on the covariance matrix. Parameter values for the first indicator of each construct, including the single indicators for Prenatal Representation of the Self, Postnatal Self-Eflicacy, and Mother-Infant Attachment, were fixed estimates, and therefore, cannot be interpreted in terms of significance testing. All other observed variables had significant loadings on their respective factors, indicating a very strong measurement model. Residual values for observed and latent variables can be found in Appendix Z. All paths between the latent constructs were also significant in the expected direction, with the exception of the paths between Maternal Representations of Attachment and Prenatal Representations of Self as Mother and between Postnatal Self- Efficacy and Mother-Infant Attachment. Thus, as hypothesized: a) more insecure, negative maternal representations of attachment were related to less secure representations of infants during pregnancy, b) more balanced representations of the infant were significantly related to more balanced representations of the self as a mother, c) more secure representations of the infant during pregnancy were related to more secure mother-infant attachment, (1) more secure or balanced representations of the self during pregnancy were related to greater feelings of self-efficacy after birth, and e) greater prenatal social support fi'om females was related to greater postnatal social support from females, which in turn was related to greater feelings of self-emcacy postnatally. Finally, ‘Residuals for the following pairs of observed variables were estimated: 22 and 1, 20 and l, 22 and 3, 6 and 4, 5 and 3, 6 and l, 3 and l, 22 and 5,18 and 15, 22 and 2, 20 and 3,14 and 22,14 and 17, 22 and 21, 21 and 6,13 and 21, 9 and 19, 20 and 21, 7and 22, 5 and 7, 7and 23, 7and 19,4and 14, 3 and 16, 5 and 21, 3 and 2]. Refer to Table 3 for the names of these variables. 99 .8. v a : one? use . E80< coma. 9 820a,.— oza ta..- aeczo cesaacomoaom .8295 scab. “55592 Emma—-3582 3.3. 585.2% 3:382 3.3% 3858“— .5502 .582 mam—om no 3265 cornucomoaom gum .8955 omega deacon. e. 5.5 9.0531 2.3 ecu .5. . can” 1 >53. .b=_=> «so—f cone. \1 5M:< uCOESOGS< MO can”. mcozficomoaom 35322 not 48.5. tenazm toga—am .023 zoom 3358; time. .cc. toaazm .535 toga—5 339.59 .852; .cezeEm 38m .8395 5&5 toga—6 €9.95 3330 .839... 628:5 22.2: E E 52:55? gee—-5502 be $82.55 EEEmom van 3355 wccoaoo Eve—2 @522 mom .N oSmE 100 more risk during pregnancy was related to less balanced or less secure prenatal representations of the infant. The iterations generally converged to show that the final model fit very well with the data. Although the x2 value of242.03 with df= 194 was significant (p = .01), x} was well below two times the degrees of fieedom. The RMSEA = .04 and the GFI = .91. Although the AGF I was .87, slightly below the cut ofl‘ of .90, several other fit indices were well above .90. Finally, the plot of standardized residuals suggested a nice fit, evidenced by small discrepancies between the observed and estimated residuals and by few outliers. See Appendix AA for the final model syntax, Appendix BB for a complete listing of goodness of fit indices, and Appendix CC for the residual plot. The original model and the final model were compared to see if the final model was significantly better than the original model. The difference in chi-square values between the two models (A X2) was 321.69, while the difi‘erence in degrees of fi'eedom (A di) was 49. This difference was statistically significant, indicating that the respecified model was significantly better than the original model. B lo to ses: Concordance Between Attachment Grou s In addition to examining the relationship between mothers’ and infants’ attachment patterns through SEM and with continuous variables, exploratory analyses were conducted to examine the concordance between mothers’ and infants’ attachment classifications over time. Although the PAAQ should theoretically yield three attachment classifications that parallel those described in the WMCI and the SS, the CFA conducted in this study showed that the PAAQ yields only one general ‘attachment security’ factor. 101 Thus, it was only possible to examine the concordance between WMCI and SS classifications. First, the concordance between the three main groups was examined: Balanced- Secure, Disengaged-Avoidant, and Distorted-Ambivalent. Because the WMCI does not have a fourth category that is parallel to the SS Disorganized classification, all disorganized infants and their mothers were dropped from these analyses. Chi-square analyses revealed no significant concordance between classifications, that is, mothers’ attachment classifications during pregnancy were not significantly related to infant attachment classifications 1 year later, when three groups were used. The overall concordance rate was 49% (kappa = .10), with 68% of Balanced mothers having Secure infants, 26% of Disengaged mothers having Avoidant infants and 22% of Distorted mothers having Ambivalent infants. See Table 9. Table 9 Concordance Between Three WMCIan_d SS Classifications WMCI Balanced Disengaged Distorted Total $ Secure 57 23 13 93 (63%) Avoidant 15 ll 5 31 (21%) Ambivalent 11 8 5 24 (16%) Total 83 (56%) 42 (28%) 23 ( 16%) 148 (100%) 1.1 = 148; 29 missing $32, 26 disorganized infants not included, 4 missing WMCI classifications. x2= 3.01, df= 4, p = .55, kappa = .10. 102 In order to increase the sample size and include the disorganized infants, all Disengaged and Distorted mothers were collapsed into an Insecure WMCI group, while all Avoidant, Ambivalent, and Disorganized infants were collapsed into an Insecure SS group. A 2 X 2 cross tabulation was then conducted to examine the concordance between Secure and Insecure groups. Results revealed a significant relationship between the WMCI and SS classifications when only two groups were used (60% concordance, kappa = .19). See Table 10 below. Thus, mothers who were classified as Secure/Balanced on the WMCI during pregnancy were more likely to have securely attached infants 1 year later, while mothers who were classified as Insecure were more likely to have insecure infants. Table 10 Concordance Between Two WMCI Ed SS Classifications Ml. Balanced Insecure Total _S__S_ Secure 57 36 93 (53%) Insecure 34 47 81 (47%) Total 91 (52%) 83 (48%) 174 (100%) n = 174; 29 missing $32, 4 missing WMCI classifications. x3= 6.47, df= 1, p -- .01, kappa = .19. 2 Although numerical scores based on the SS were estimated for missing subjects in the SEM analyses described above, overall classifications were not estimated and thus were not included in these analyses. It was believed that estimating classifications posed a greater risk to data interpretation if errors were made. 103 CHAPTER 7: DISCUSSION This study was designed to test Stern’s (1995) theory about the psychological processes involved in the transition to motherhood, as well as the effects of both intrapsychic and environmental factors on mother-infant attachment as described in the attachment literature. It was hypothesized that: a) mothers’ representations of attachment would impact representations of the infant and of the self as a mother, b) the latter two sets of representations would be related to one another but remain distinct constructs, c) representations of the infant during pregnancy would be related to mother-infant attachment at 1 year, (I) representations of the selfas a mother would be related to postnatal self-eflicacy, which would in turn, be related to mother-infant attachment, e) social support fiom females would be related to mother-infant attachment through its impact on self-emcacy, and t) maternal risk factors would negatively impact mother- infant attachment at 1 year. In general, the results from this study supported most of the hypotheses, and thus, provided support for Stern’s theory as well as prior findings in the attachment literature. Results will be discussed in more detail next, followed by a discussion of this study’s strengths and limitations, future directions for research, and clinical implications of the results. Re resentatio ocesses Structural equation modeling results revealed a significant relationship between mothers’ representations of attachment, based on recollections of the relationships with their own mothers, and mothers’ representations of their infants during pregnancy. Mothers who reported more negative, insecure attachment experiences characterized by, for example, more rejection, greater feelings of anger, less love, and trouble remembering 104 events from childhood (believed to be a defensive process involved in insecure attachment) had more insecure representations of their infants during pregnancy. These insecure representations were characterized by not only less sensitive and accepting attitudes towards the infant, but more impoverished, rigid, and incoherent narratives. These results are consistent with previous empirical and clinical papers, which have suggested a strong relationship between mothers’ own attachment experiences during childhood and mothers’ attachment representations of their own infants (Ammaniti, 1991; George & Solomon, 1996; Fraiberg et al., 1975; Slade & Cohen, 1996). However, this is the first study to demonstrate this relationship prenatally y; empirically, rather than through case studies or with postnatal samples. At this time, it is still somewhat unclear how mothers’ attachment experiences influence their representations of their infants, and specifically attachment-related representations, but some researchers have speculated about the nature of this relationship. In one of the earliest papers, Fraiberg et al. (1975) suggested that mothers who have not had their own attachment needs met are unable to be empathically attuned to their infants because their own needs interfere with their ability to do so. In other words, mothers with insecure attachment experiences may not be able to decrease their own subjectivity for the purpose of being acutely in tune with the infant or may not be able to make psychological room for the infant, which Stern (1995) proposes are important transformations that ideally occur during pregnancy. As a result, they may be unable to think about and discuss their infants in a non-distorted, coherent, and flexible way. Similarly, Fonagy et al. (1991, 1993) have proposed that mothers who have not had attachment needs met do not develop the ability to understand the mental states of self 105 and others (lacking ‘reflective self-function’), which leads to greater difiiculty anticipating their infant’s needs, ultimately leading to less sensitive caregiving. Furthermore, Fonagy et al. stated that mothers who are more defensive in their thinking about attachment (e.g., display idealization or repression) are less able to understand or be in tune with their infant’s mental world. Clearly, there is much to be learned and these are all interesting hypotheses to examine in future research. Surprisingly, model results did not reveal a significant relationship between mothers’ representations of attachment and mothers’ representations of themselves as mothers during pregnancy. At first glance, this appears to suggest that the way in which mothers see themselves as caregivers during pregnancy is not influenced by mothers’ experiences of being cared for during childhood, which is contrary to Stern’s (1995) theory and to prior literature (Ammaniti, 1991; Ammaniti et al. 1992; Fava Vizziello et al., 1993; Slade & Cohen, 1996). However, this is not believed to be the case. First, bivariate correlations (see Appendix R) in this study indicated that many of the observed variables which measure maternal representations of attachment were, in fact, significantly related to prenatal representations of the selfas mother. That is, more positive, secure experiences dm'ing childhood were related to more secure representations of the self, characterized by a confident and realistic view of the selfas a caregiver. In addition, model results indicated that there was a very strong, significant relationship between representations of the infant and representations of the selfas a mother, as hypothesized. In fact, this relationship was almost perfect, with more secure representations of the infant related to more secure or balanced representations of the self. Thus, because the two representational constructs (infant and self) were so highly related 106 and shared so much variance, only one significant pathway fi'om maternal representations of attachment to representations of the infant emerged. That is, the SEM analyses parceled out the shared variance and suggested that this was the best model fit for the data. There is also an important theoretical explanation for these results that should be considered. Because there was a near perfect relationship between representations of the infant and representations of the selfas mother, it is possible that these sets of representations are not distinct, as hypothesized, but rather are part of one integrated “caregiving” representational system as suggested by George and Solomon (1989, 1996). These researchers suggest, as others have (e.g., Bowlby, 1973; Bretherton & Munholland, 1999; Slade & Cohen, 1996), that representations of the selfand other are related and develop fiom working models of early attachment experiences. However, they seem to take this one step further and imply that in the context of an attachment relationship and from the parental perspective these representations are integrated into one representatioml system. Thus, representations of the self as a caregiver are integrally related to representations of the infant and these should not be considered distinct sets of representations. In other words, the internal working model of caregiving includes all information related to the role as attachment figure, including both information about the self and information about the child-to-be-cared-for. Interestingly, George and Solomon are the only researchers to discuss the idea of an integrated caregiving representation in such detail, and their published papers have only described this process in parents of early school-age children. The results from the present study suggest that an integrated caregiving representational system may begin much earlier during pregnancy. 107 As hypothesized, results also revealed that prenatal representations of the infant were significantly related to infant attachment security 1 year postpartum. More secure or balanced maternal representations during pregnancy, characterized by richness, coherence and sensitivity for example, were related to greater infant attachment security, while less secure representations were related to less infant security. These results are consistent with several other studies, which have demonstrated a significant relationship between representations of the child from the parental perspective and child attachment security (Bretherton et al., 1989; George & Solomon, 1989, 1996). More importantly, however, these results provide powerful evidence that representations of the infant before the infant is even born may significantly impact infant attachment secm’ity one year later. Only one previous study has examined this relationship prospectively (Benoit et al., 1997) and this was in a low-risk, middle-class sample, while the present study included more high-risk participants. These findings have important clinical implications for working with at-risk pregnant women, which will be discussed in more detail below. Because representations of the self during pregnancy and postnatal self-efficacy were examined for the caregiving role specifically, it was hypothesized that these would be significantly related. Results confirmed this hypothesis; more secure or balanced representations of the self were related to greater reported feelings of postnatal self- efficacy. However, unexpectedly, postnatal self-emcacy was not significantly related to infant attachment security. There are several possible explanations for this finding. One explanation is that self-reported feelings of efficacy are not actually related to infant attachment, as measured through the well-known Strange Situation procedure. Prior studies that have demonstrated a clear relationship between self-efficacy and 108 “attachment” have measured both constructs through maternal self-report (Mercer & Ferketick, 1990, 1994; Williams etal., 1987), and thus, may have only actually demonstrated a relationship between self-reported efficacy and self-reported beliefs about the mother-child relationship. That is, the findings from these studies may have been spurious due to the complete reliance on maternal self-report and/or may have been misinterpreted. Another possibility is that there is in fact a significant relationship between mothers’ feelings of self-efficacy in the caregiving role and infant attachment security, but that mothers were unable to accurately self-report about themselves in this domain. Studies that have examined the impact of mothers’ representations of themselves as mothers, as coded by trained researchers, on mother-infant attachment (as measured through laboratory procedures like the Strange Situation) have found significant relationships (Ammaniti, 1991; George & Solomon, 1996). One study that examined the relationship between self-reported eflicacy and mother-infant attachment in the Strange Situation reported that self-efficacy ratings did not differ among mothers of secure and avoidant children (Spieker & Booth, 1988), and the authors reasoned that mothers of avoidant children were defensively reporting high levels of self-eficacy, which is consistent with defensive processes observed in mothers of avoidant infants. Support for this explanation fiom the current study comes fiom the observation that maternal self- eficacy scores were highly skewed towards the positive end, which would not be theoretically expected fiom such a high-risk sample unless there was defensive reporting. The studies conducted by Donovan and colleagues (1989, 1990) also provide support for this explanation. They found that some mothers displayed high illusions of 109 control, or unrealistically positive views of themselves as caregivers, which appeared to mask feelings of inadequacy. Furthermore, mothers with high illusions of control were more likely to have insecurely attached children in the Strange Situation. Again, these results suggest that mothers may not be able to accurately self-report on caregiving efficacy. Although results from the present study did show a significant relationship between prenatal representations of the self (coded by researchers) and postnatal self- eficacy, self-efficacy scores may not have been ‘accurate’ enough to predict infant attachment security. It is possflale that had researchers coded representations of the self as a mother fi'om a clinical interview postnatally (like the WMCI), representations would have been significantly related to infant security in the expected direction This may be a more valid way of measuring maternal self-eficacy than self-report questionnaires. One final possibility mentioned here is that there were problem in the measurement model that may have prevented a true significant relationship from emerging between these constructs. Both constructs, postnatal self-efficacy and infant attachment, only had one indicator each. This creates possible identification problems and there is less variance to be explained for each construct, making it harder to detect relationships. As already mentioned, the observed variable for maternal self-efficacy also did not have adequate variance. In sum, modeling results indicated that mothers’ representations of their own attachment, of their infants, and of themselves as mothers were related to each other and to infant attachment security in theoretically-important ways. Perhaps the most important pathways in this part of the model were between maternal representations of attachment, representations of the infant, and infant attachment security at 1 year. These results 110 demonstrated that attachment representations may be transmitted from mother to child, as prior research has suggested. That is, more secure representations of attachment fiom childhood predict more secure representations of the infant during pregnancy, which in turn predict more secure infant attachment after birth. It is also likely that representations of the self as a mother are involved in this pathway, as part of a broader caregiving representation. These findings extend the results from previous studies by showing some continuity of attachment over time, beginning in pregnancy, in a more diverse and high- risk sample. Social Summrt and Mother-Infant Attachment According to Stern (1995), social support fiom females is particularly helpfiil to women as they transition to motherhood, e.g., by providing advice and emotional support. Therefore, social support was expected to indirectly impact mother-infant attachment through its effects on self-eficacy. As hypothesized, prenatal support from other women was significantly related to postnatal support from women. That is, the quality of participants’ social support remained relatively stable over time. In addition, the perceived quality of support was significantly related to self-efficacy, with better support related to greater feelings of self-reported efficacy. These findings are consistent with previous studies showing a significant relationship between social support and maternal self-efficacy (Cutrona & Troutman, 1986; Shea & Tronick, 1988; Teti et al., 1996). It also supports Crittenden’s (1985) conclusion that quality of support may be more important than quantity of support, since the measure in this study was average satisfaction rather than simply the number of supporters in participants’ networks. These findings suggest that social support can enhance feelings of efficacy by, for example, 111 increasing parenting skills and knowledge, providing confirmation or assurances of parenting capabilities, or providing opportunities to model others. Support from women specifically, many of whom have likely been mothers, may be especially helpful because of the increased credibility granted to them because of their own ermeriences with caregiving. Thus, advice or support fi'om women may be given more ‘weight’ than support from men, at least in the context of pregnancy and parenthood. Indeed, this was the first study to show empirically that support from women had a significant impact on maternal feelings of self-eficacy, as Stern (1995) has suggested theoretically. As mentioned previously, however, maternal self-efficacy was unrelated to infant attachment security, and thus, it appears that social support did not have an indirect impact on infant attachment, as was hypothesized in the current study. There are several possible explanations for this finding. It may be that social support does in fact have an indirect effect on infant attachment through self-efficacy, as has been shown by a few other researchers (Donovan & Leavitt, 1989; Isabella, 1994), but this indirect relationship did not emerge because of the problems described above with the self-eficacy construct and the pathway between self-efficacy and infant attachment in the current study. It may also be that social support has an indirect impact on mother-infant attachment, but through its effects on some other variable such as materml behaviors or parenting, e.g., sensitivity, which were not examined in this study. It also seems possible that social support might have a direct impact on infant attachment security, as others have found (Crnic et al., 1986; Crockenberg, 1981). However, when all other variables were taken into account, the data in the current study 112 did not support this notion (although emotioml and caregiving support were weakly related correlationally to infant attachment security, see Appendix R). Rather, the results from this study suggest that if social support has an impact on mother-infant attachment, it is probably more indirect than direct. This conclusion is supported by a few other studies that have reported no direct relationship between social support and attachment (Belsky & Isabella, 1988; Zeanah et al., 1993). In sum, the results fiom this study generally do not clarify the already-mixed findings in the literature about the relationship between social support and mother-infant attachment, although the results do support the idea that social support positively afl‘ects women’s self-eficacy in the maternal role. It will be important for future studies to continue to examine the role that support may or may not have in contributing to child attachment security, including different dimensions of support such as type and source of support. The Impact of Risk Factors It was hypothesized that maternal risk factors (e.g., domestic violence, depression, poverty) would directly predict mother-infant attachment. Although risk factors were related to infant attachment security correlationally (see Appendix R), with more risk related to greater insecurity, risk was more strongly related to prenatal representations of the infant when all variables were considered simultaneously. Therefore, SEM results revealed a significant pathway fi'om risk status to prenatal representations of the infant rather than fi'om risk status to mother-infant attachment at 1 year. Women who experienced more risk factors such as domestic violence, depression, and demographic risks had more insecure representations of their infants during pregnancy, characterized 113 by less sensitivity, acceptance, and less open, rich, and coherent narratives. Representations were in turn related to mother-infant attachment. This finding (risk related to representations) was not originally hypothesized, however, the results make theoretical sense. Although prior studies have shown a direct relationship between risk factors and mother-infant attachment in the Strange Situation (Martins & Gafl‘an, 2000; Spieker & Booth, 1988; Zeanah et al. 1999), similar to the correlational results in this study, it has generally been assumed that there is some intervening step in this process. That is, maternal exposure to risk itselfdoes not ‘automatically’ create less securely attached children. Rather, risk such as maternal depression or poverty may, for example, lead to less sensitive or more rejecting behaviors (Gelfand & Teti, 1990; Halpern, 1993; Norton, 1990), which are then related to the development of less secure attachment. The findings fiom this study support the notion that risk is more indirectly related to mother-infant attachment through its effects on the mother’s functioning and way of thinking about her child (Thompson, 2000). This indirect relationship between risk and child fimctioning via maternal functioning has already been shown in other areas of the literature examining difl‘erent child outcomes, e.g., anxiety, peer relationships, or school fitnctioning (e.g., Holden & Ritchie, 1991; Levendosky & Graham-Bermann, 2001). Thus, it appears this indirect relationship may be true for risk and infant attachment as well. These results are also consistent with one recent study showing a relationship betweenmaternalriskandmaternalrepresentationsofthe childandofthe selfasa mother (Pajulo, Savonlahti, Sourander, Piha, & Helenius, 2001). In this study, results revealed that maternal risks such as substance use, depression, and demographic risks, 114 were significantly related to the content of maternal representations of self and child during pregnancy. In this study, participants were categorized into a high- or low-risk group based on the presence or absence of a risk factor and comparisons were made on responses to a self-report questionnaire. Findings indicated that women in the high-risk group rated their infant and themselves as mothers much more negatively than the low- risk group. Unfortunately, this study did not examine the relationship between prenatal representations and infant attachment after birth and was limited by the sole use of self- report data. In contrast, the results of the current study extend and improve upon those findings by Pajulo et aL by showing that maternal risks affect not only the content of prenatal representations, but also the way in which mothers talk about their infants, the latter of which may be particularly important for determining the overall quality of attachment. E lorato ses Exploratory analyses were conducted to examine the concordance between mothers’ and infants’ attachment classifications over time using the WMCI and SS classifications. This was believed to be another way of examining the continuity or discontinuity of attachment over time between mother and infant. When disorganized infants were omitted from analyses and when three groups were used, results revealed a concordance or match rate of 49% between WMCI and SS classifications, which was non-significant. When two groups were used (Insecure and Secure), the concordance rate increased to 60%. This two by two analysis was significant; however, the kappa was still very low (.19). Overall, these results indicate a significant amount of discontinuity between mothers’ attachment representations of the infant and mother-infant attachment 115 at 1 year, especially discontinuity of insecure attachment classifications. Thus, although modeling results revealed a strong, significant relationship between these variables when operationalized as dimensional constructs, categorical analyses revealed a substantial amount of discontinuity as well. Although the lower rates of concordance may be partly due to measurement error (imperfect inter-rater reliability), it is more likely that there is a theoretical explanation for these findings. In fact, theoretical and empirical discussions of attachment discontinuity have been rapidly increasing within the last several years, and these seem to be particularly relevant to the current study. Investigators have examined the stability or instability of attachment in three different ways: within the same groups of infants over time, within the same groups of individuals from infancy to adulthood, and within caregiver-child dyads. In all three areas, researchers have generally found several consistent results: 1) there is less continuity among the insecure forms of attachment, 2) there is less continuity in ' -risk samples compared to low-risk samples, and 3) changes in family circumstances, and especially those that have broad effects on multiple family members, are most likely to alter attachment security or insecurity (see Thompson, 2000 for a review). Each of these areas in the literature will be discussed in more detail next. Egeland and Farber (1984) were among the first to examine attachment stability in young children over time. They found that 60% of their high-risk infant sample had stable classifications from 12 to 18 months of age, with more stability in the secure group (74%) than in the insecure groups (45% for Avoidant and 37% for Ambivalent). Correlates of infant change away from security (moving fi'om secure to insecure groups) were less maternal education, more maternal aggression and suspiciousness, more life 116 stress, and single parenthood. Correlates of change towards security (moving fiom inmoure groups to secure group) were greater maternal effectiveness and responsiveness, better understanding of the infant, and more advanced infant development. Similarly, Frodi, Grolnick, and Bridges (1985) found an overall stability rate of 66% in infant attachment classification from 12 to 20 months, but this was predominately due to stability of security (77%) rather than insecurity (33% for Avoidant and 20% for Ambivalent groups). More recently, Vondraet al. (1999) reported a 50% stability rate from 12 to 18 months in their low-income sample with 68% stable Secure, 25% stable Avoidant, 33% stable Ambivalent, and 46% stable Disorganized. Correlates of stable security or move toward security were relatively low risk experiences, low scores on aggressive/suspicious personality, low stressful events, and high relationship satisfaction. Correlates of stable insecurity or move toward insecurity were maternal anxiety and depression, infant difficult temperament, low relationship satisfaction, stressful life events, and self-reported anger. In a follow-up study, Vondra et al. (2001) reported an overall increase in insecurity at 24 months compared to 12 and 18 month classifications. Although the overall percentage of stability was similar at 24 months as it was at 18 months (45% and 50%, respectively), there was less stability within the Avoidant and Ambivalent insecure groups fiom 12/18 months to 24 months: 89% stable Secure, 61% stable Avoidant, 32% stable Ambivalent, and 100% stable Disorganized (n = 2 in the latter group). Thus, overall, these and other studies examining infant attachment stability (e.g., Barnett et al., 1999) have generally found less stability of insecure groups and less stability in high-risk samples. 117 Several recent studies have also examined the stability of attachment over many years fi'om early childhood to adulthood. For example, Waters et al. (2000) followed a middle-class sample fiom infancy to early adulthood and found that 64% of the sample had the same classification at both time points. Individuals who experienced negative life events such as parental loss, abuse, or life-threatening illness to a family member were significantly more likely to change attachment classifications over time, especially to change from secure to insecure rather than fi'om one form of insecurity to another. The authors concluded that the relatively high degree of stability in this sample may have been due to the low-risk nature of the sample. Similarly, Hamilton (2000) reported a 63% stability rate fi'om infiincy to adolescence. However, this study found that negative life events were significantly associated with the maintenance of insecure attachment over time. Finally, Weinfield, Sroufe, and Egeland (2000) reported a 39% stability rate from infancy to early adulthood in their low-income, ' -risk sample. These researchers found that stressful life events were unrelated to stability or change, but that child maltreatment was related to stability of insecurity and maternal depression was related to movement toward insecurity fi'orn security. Overall, these three studies suggest that, although there may be continuity in attachment, there can also be change in attachment over time and discontinuity is often related to changes in the home environment. Finally, there have been a number of studies examining the transmission or stability of attachment from caregiver to child, many of which have been described in detail in the current paper’s literature review. Several studies have shown, for example, more discontinuity among the insecure attachment classifications when comparing mothers’ representations of attachment on the AAI and infant attachment in the SS 118 (Benoit & Parker, 1994); this may be particularly true for the preoccupied-ambivalent groups (Fonagy et al., 1991; Pederson et al., 1998; Zeanah et al., 1993). Other studies examining the relationship between mothers’ representations of the infant and infant attachment security, similar to the present study, have also found less stability among insecure attachment groups (Benoit et al., 1997; Zeanah et al., 1994). OveralL the results from the present study’s exploratory analyses are consistent with other findings, which show less stability in insecure types of attachment and in high- risk samples. That is, the results fiom this study found less stability between WMCI and SS categories for the two insecure attachment groups compared to the secure group, as well as lower overall concordance rates (49% using three groups and 60% using two groups) compared to previous studies with low-risk samples. Findings related to the discontinuity of attachment classifications do not necessarily contradict earlier SEM results. First, when categorical attachment data are used, there is a potential for some masking of continuity compared to when continuous data are used (Weinfield et al., 2000). This may be because there are differences in felt security within the broad classifications and/or because of the greater likelihood of classification error for individuals that do not fit neatly into a group, e.g., individuals that are on the border between two groups. Alternatively, the findings related to discontinuity actually provide a more complete picture of attachment theory. Attachment theory predicts relative stability over time; however, it also posits that internal working models, and development in general, remain open to revision in the context of new life experiences (Waters et al., 2000). That is, attachment theory predicts both stability and change. More recent work in this area, 119 described earlier, has shown that change is more likely to occur in individuals who live in less stable, more chaotic environments and/or who experience more profound life changes. Results fiom the present study are based on a high-risk sample of individuals, who likely experience less stable living conditions, and this study examined stability before and after a major life event, the birth of a child. Thus, it is not surprising that there was significant discontinuity in attachment classifications. Sm of Resglts In sum, results based on SEM analyses generally supported Stern’s (1995) theory about the psychological processes related to the transition to motherhood. One of the most striking results fi'om model testing was the continuity of attachment between mothers’ representations of their own attachment to mothers’ representations of the infant during pregnancy to infant attachment at 1 year, when attachment was operatiomlized as a dimensional construct. Thus, mothers appeared to think about their infants in ways that were similar to the way they thought about being cared for during childhood, which in turn, was related to the quality of attachment they formed with their infants after birth In addition, the results supported the notion that there may be an integrated caregiving representational system that women develop during pregnancy, which includes representations of both the infant and the self as a caregiver. Social support fiom females also predicted maternal self-efficacy, as expected, but self-efficacy was not significantly related to infant attachment. It is believed that this lack of finding was due to particular measurement and statistical reasons, rather than to fiaulty theory. Thus, it cannot be concluded that social support and maternal self-efficacy do not impact infant attachment. 120 Interestingly, the data suggested that maternal risk factors impact mother-infant attachment indirectly through their impact on maternal representations of the child, rather than directly. Thus, maternal representations appear to be an important mechanism through which psychosocial risk exerts its effects. Exploratory analyses revealed substantial discontinuity of attachment when attachment was assessed categorically. This is not surprising in light of the high-risk nature of the sample and what has recently been reported in the literature about discontinuity in high-risk and insecure groups. In addition, model results and exploratory results provided evidence that attachment is both relatively stable over time and is capable of being revised in light of life events and circumstances. There are a number of strengths in the current study that support previous findings and expand the current state of the attachment literature. First, this study used a prospective design, assessing maternal representations and other variables before the infant was even born. This type of design adds more confidence to the conclusion that prenatal risk factors and prenatal representations contribute to infant attachment security, rather than vice versa In addition, this study is one of the few studies to examine the impact of both intrapsychic and environmental factors in relation to mother-infant attachment, making this more comprehensive than most attachment studies. Similarly, this study examined these processes within a high-risk sample, which has been done much less frequently than studies with low-risk, middle class fiimilies. It is important to show that much of Stern’s (1995) theory, as well as basic ideas fi'om attachment theory, hold true in this population as well as in higher functioning populations. 121 There are also several methodological strengths. Although several variables were measured through maternal self-report, several other constructs were assessed using clinical interviews and observational methods that were coded by trained, reliable coders. This minimizes the possibility of spurious findings, especially those findings that involve variables that are not based on self-report. More importantly, these methods for assessing attachment are believed to provide more rich and valid data because they allow for measurement of unconscious or automatic processes (Crowell et al., 1999), which are believed to be intimately involved in attachment-related representations. In other words, one does not have to be concerned that the participant is self-reporting inaccurately about their attachment-related thoughts and feelings. Finally, the use of structural equation modeling provides several advantages for data analysis and interpretation. It is generally a more stringent method of testing relationships between variables because it estimates parameters after taking into account all other relationships and shared variance. In addition, measurement error can be estimated and therefore removed so that resulting relationships between constructs are error fiee. It also allows for the testing of a complex model (including possible modifications) by providing overall fit indices and evaluating relationships simultaneously, rather than requiring a series of analyses to test bivariate relationships. However, this study was not without limitations, and it is important to consider these limitations when drawing conclusions about the results. First, the use of self-report questionnaires to assess some of the constructs in the study poses some risk for inaccurate andbiasedreporting. Thishasalreadybeendiscussed fortheself-reportusedtomeasure maternal self-eflicacy, which was highly skewed toward positive responses. This 122 particular measure may have been problematic because each item begins with the statement “How good are you at. ...” followed by some description of caregiving. It seems that this wording may be especially prone to social desirability. Although the self- report used to assess materml representations of attachment experiences fiom childhood (PAAQ) predicted other constructs in the expected direction, some attachment researchers would argue that self-report questionnaires are unable to accurately assess attachment representations. Although it is true that the PAAQ does not yield some of the process-oriented constructs related to defensiveness (e.g., coherence in the narrative) and to overall attachment classifications, the results suggest that this instrument may be valid for assessing overall attachment security, possrbly through some of the subscales such as ‘No Memory ’. Another disadvantage of using self-report questionnaires with the same informant is the possibility of spurious or inflated findings, for example, between maternal social support and maternal self-eflicacy. Fortunately, most pathways in the model included constructs that were measured using two difierent methods, e. g., self-report, interview codes, and observation codes. Another limitation that was more statistical in nature was the fact that three constructs had only one indicator, which is generally not advised when conducting structural model testing. Single indicator constructs present possible identification problems and may make it more difficult to detect relationships because there is less variance to work with for these constructs. Finally, although the high-risk nature of the current sample was generally a strength of the study (since most previous studies have only included low-risk, middle-class families), it also limits the generalizability of findings. Thus, the results fiom this particular study may only be 123 representative of women that are generally more at-risk (e. g., low-income, single parents, history of domestic violence) than of women who are generally not at-risk. Directions for Future Research There are a number of ways that future research can expand our current understanding of the processes related to the formation of mother-infant attachment, including the influence of both intrapsychic and environmental variables. First, it seems that more research needs to be done in diverse and higher-risk groups of mothers and infants. Some researchers are studying attachment in high-risk groups such as low- income and maltreatment samples (e.g., Barnett et al., 1999;Ege1and & Sroufe, 1984; Spieker & Booth, 1988; Vondra et al., 1999; Weinfield et al., 2000); however, the majority of attachment research is done with low-risk, middle-class, Caucasian samples. It also seems important for future studies to replicate the findings here in order to further validate (or invalidate) Stern’s (1995) theory about the transition to motherhood and the role of female social support in facilitating this transition. This is the first model to test his theory and others should continue to test his theory with different populations. A very important, more specific direction for future research is the further examination of processes related to the stability and instability of attachment, both within the same individual and across generations fiom parent to child. Most attachment research has focused on validating and emphasizing the notion of continuity over time, but more work needs to be done to investigate cases of discontinuity and factors related to discontinuity. As others have begun to point out more clearly, Bowlby (1973, 1980) not only indicated that attachment is somewhat continuous and resistant to change, but that it is also dynamic and open to revision. Thus, there needs to be further examination 124 of factors such as stressful life events, changes in the family environment, changes in caregiver behaviors, etc... to determine how significant life experiences may alter attachment representations. In addition, it will be important to determine what experiences are important for what types of people and at what developmental stages. Thompson (2000) recently suggested that life events which affect relational patterns and developmental periods during which there are significant representational advances may be particularly important areas to investigate. Clearly, this work has just begun and will have pivotal implications for clinical work. Another important direction for future research is the investigation of intermediate factors that might further elucidate the link between maternal representations and infant attachment, e.g., between the AAI or WMCI and the Strange Situation. The results from this study suggested that there is a strong relationship between materml representations during pregnancy and infant attachment security afler birth, when measured as dimensional constructs, but it is still unclear how these representations are “transmitted” to the infant. Although the majority of prior research in this area has focused on maternal sensitivity and responsiveness (through overt, observable behaviors) as the mediating variable, research has shown that sensitivity explains only a relatively small amount of the relationship between mothers’ representations and infant attachment (DeWolff & van Ijzendoom, 1997; Pederson et al., 1998; van Ijzendoom, 1995). Thus, other observable variables, which may reflect important aspects of the caregiver as a secure base, need to be investigated. Finally, there are several methodological considerations for future attachment research. First, it has been suggested by several attachment researchers (Cicchetti, 2001; 125 Weinfield et al., 2000) that both dimensional and categorical measures of attachment be used in future research Each way of operationalizing attachment may be useful and may be complementary to the other. This idea is supported by the current findings, which revealed an interesting picture of attachment processes when both kinds of data were used. There has been some debate over whether and how to convert the Strange Situation codes into a continuous variable. One method is the conversion system developed by Bretherton and colleagues (1989, 1990) used in the present study, which appears to be a viable option. An additional method that should be considered for firture research is the empirically derived system described by Richters, Waters, and Vaughn (1988), which uses the subscale scores that are coded and used to make overall classifications. This method was shown by the authors to be a reliable and valid means of assessing attachment security along a continuum. Last, but not least, it is important for firture research to use longitudinal, prospective designs to examine the complex processes involved in the development and evolution of attachment over time. Clinical Implications Finally, the results fiom the present study not only extend our conceptual and theoretical understanding of the processes related to the development of mother-infant attachment, they also provide important information that is directly relevant to clinical work with at-risk pregnant and parenting women. A number of researchers and clinicians have already developed treatments for work with parents and infants (e. g., Lieberman & Pawl, 1993; McDonough, 1993; Stern, 1995); however, results fiom studies such as this one can help guide and support the direction of these treatments. 126 First, the results of this study suggested that maternal risk factors such as poverty and domestic violence have an indirect impact on infant attachment through its effects on maternal representations of the infant. Maternal social support may also have an indirect impact on infant attachment, although this was less clear. Still, it seems important for clinical interventions to consider and attend to environmental factors that could be impinging on a mother’s ability to psychologically prepare for motherhood and her ability to think about her infant and herself as a mother in a non—defensive, sensitive, and accepting manner. Thus, clinicians may need to evaluate the mother’s fiimily, community, and social systems in order to help the mother correct or alter those factors that may be interfering with the caregiving process, even as this process begins during pregnancy. The most striking finding in this study that is directly relevant to clinical work is the strong, direct relationship between prenatal representations of the infant and infant attachment at 1 year. These findings suggest that early interventions with at-risk pregnant women can be critical in preventing the transmission of insecure attachment from mother to child. Furthermore, these results indicate that clinical work must address mothers’ representational processes including representations of their own attachment and representations of their infant and themselves as caregivers. Fortunately, many of the existing parent-infant psychotherapy programs do focus on altering mothers’ representations as a way to improve mother-infant relationships (Lieberman & Pawl, 1993; Stern, 1995); however, these programs are generally geared towards working with mothers after the birth of the baby. It is important for research studies like this one to 127 show empirical support for these types of programs, as well as the importance of beginning this work during pregnancy. Programs do, however, difi‘er in how they approach altering mothers’ representations. Stern (1995) summarizes a number of programs which all use different “ports of entry” to alter parental representations, but states that all share the common goal of changing these representations. Working with the assumption that mothers’ representations influence behaviors towards the infant which in turn influence infant behaviors and infant representations (and vice versa), he states that clinicians can focus on any of these elements as a way of entering into and altering the mothers’ representations. For example, some programs use infant behaviors as a starting point, while others use maternal behaviors, etc. . .Subsequent to altering representations, it is believed that mothers will interact with their infants in a more positive, sensitive, and responsive manner, which eventually leads to more secure infant attachment. Based on the current study, it seems that intervening through the mothers’ representations would be the natural starting point when working with pregnant women since parent behaviors could not yet be observed. Finally, in addition to exploring mothers’ representations directly, an equally important part of this clinical work includes providing the mother with a “corrective attachment experience,” whereby the therapist serves as an attachment figure who is internalized and integrated into existing representations. Through both exploration and experience, the mother is able to alter representations of self and other, enabling her to be a more secure attachment figure to her own infant. 128 Conclusions In sum, this study expands our current understanding of the processes related to the formation of mother-infant attachment, beginning in pregnancy and extending into the first year of life. This study provides empirical support for the significance of both individual, intrapsychic factors and environmental factors in predicting infant attachment, which demonstrates the importance of studying mother-infant attachment from an ecological perspective, particularly in diverse, high-risk samples. The results fi'om this study suggest that maternal representations during pregnancy and maternal risk factors may be especially important predictors of infant attachment security, while the role of maternal social support is less clear. Interestingly, the results fi'om this study also demonstrate that there is both continuity and discontinuity of attachment over time, which provides empirical support for both aspects of attachment theory. In addition to the theoretical implications, the results fi'om this study have clear clinical implications; results suggest that it is critical to preventively intervene with women at risk for attachment disturbances during pregnancy, by targeting both intrapsychic (e.g., representations) and environmental factors. Finally, future research in this area can expand our understanding of the complex processes involved in the evolution of attachment over time by: 1) operationalizing attachment both dimensionally and categorically, 2) examining the stability and instability of attachment within and across individuals, and 3) examining possible nechanisms at multiple system levels, e. g., individual and cormnunity, that account for this (in)stability. In so doing, firture research will also help deepen and broaden our understanding of more general adaptive and maladaptive developmental processes throughout the life span. 129 APPENDICES 130 APPENDIX A Flyers for Recruitment ARE YOU PREGNANT? YOU MAY BE ELIGIBLE TO PARTICIPATE IN A STUDY ABOUT MOTHER-INF ANT RELATIONSHIPS :2 $50.00 1!, We are looking for pregnant women to participate in a research study at Michigan State University. You will be asked about experiences and feelings during pregnancy, perceptions of your infants, and recent life events. - Interview can be done at MSU or at your home. - You will be paid $50.00 in cash. a All information is kept completely confidential. !! $50.00 1! ******************************************** If you are interested or would like more information, please call 432-1447 and ask for Dr. Anne Bogat’s Mother-Infant Study MOTHER- INFANT STUDY 432-1447 MOTHER- INFANT STUDY 432-1447 MOTHER- INFANT STUDY 432-1447 MOTHER- INFANT STUDY 432-1447 432-1447 131 HAVE YOU BEEN HURT BY SOMEONE YOU LOVE? ARE YOU PREGNANT AND HAVE YOU BEEN PUSHED OR GRABBED OR HIT OR SLAPPED OR KICKED (OR WORSE) BY A PARTNER OR BOYFRIEND DURING YOUR PREGNANCY? We need women to take part in an interview about their lives and their pregnancies. - Interview can be done at MSU or at your home. - You will be paid $50.00 in cash. - All information is kept completely confidential, !! $50.00 1! ******************************************** Ifyou are interested or would like more information, please call 432-1447 and ask for Dr. Anne Bogat’s Mother-Infant Study MOTHER- MOTHER- MOTHER- MOTHER- INFANT INFANT INFANT INFANT STUDY STUDY STUDY STUDY 432-1447 432-1447 432-1447 4 2-1447 132 APPENDIX B Time 1 Consent Form MOTHER-INFANT STUDY CONSENT FORM This study is part of a survey of women in Michigan, some of whom may be experiencing domestic violence. We hope to learn about the strengths that you bring to your situation, your feelings and perceptions of your baby during pregnancy, and your relationships with others, including family members, partners, and fi'iends. We hope to use this information to help plan better programs for families experiencing domestic violence. If you decide to take part in the survey, you will be asked questions about how you have been feeling recently, events that have happened to you, your feelings about pregnancy and your baby, the people in your life who provide support to you, and your memories of your childhood. This will take a total of 2-3 hours. All information will be kept strictly confidential. Your name will be removed from all questionnaires and an identification number will be put on them instead. All questionnaires will be kept in locked file cabinets in a locked office. Your identity will not be revealed in any reports written about this study. We will summarize information from all study participants and will not report information about individuals. The only exception is in the case of ongoing child abuse. If you indicate that child abuse is occurring in your household, we are required to make a report to Protective Services. You have the right to withdraw fi'om this study at any point during the interview with no penalty or negative consequences. Your decision about whether to participate or not will not affect your relationship with any agencies or Michigan State University. If you have any questions, please ask us. If you have questions later, you can contact Dr. Anne Bogat or Dr. Alytia Levendosky at (517) 432-1447. We are also interested in recontacting you about 2 months after the birth of your baby by telephone and then we would like to meet with you and your baby at 12 months after the birth of your baby. So at the end of the interview today, we will ask you for information that will help us keep in contact with you. Your participation at gig time does not obligate you to participate in the second telephone appointment, or the third interview. You will be paid $50 for the first interview, mailed a baby gift after the telephone interview, and $75 and a baby gift for the third interview, if you wish to participate. A. L A_-__A__Aa LAAAALAAAAAAAAAAAL¢AALAAAA. A A _A__A_A__A__.__‘_.A__A_ A. LL; L... A LA A. A. L... LAAAAAAAAAJ ALAAAJ AAA TTTTTTTTTT rTTTT-r‘r‘r-r r‘V'TTTT'rfv—f-TVTTV 'v T'I'TT‘V'T1 TVTTTTT‘V‘TTT‘TTTTw—w ‘v I 77* 1 1 TVTTTVVT-rf’r‘r I have read and understood the above statanents. I understand that my participation in this study is completely voluntary and that I can withdraw from this study at any time without penalty or negative consequences. Signature of Participant: Date: Witness: Date: Anne Bogat, Ph.D. Alytia Levendosky, Ph.D. Michigan State University Michigan State University Department of Psychology Department of Psychology 123 Snyder Hall 121 Snyder Hall East Lansing,MI48824-lll7 EastLansing,MI48824-lll7 133 APPENDIX C Time 2 Verbal Assent Form Before I begin asking you questions, I need to get your verbal consent to participate in this interview. I will read a few paragraphs and ask you whether you understand and agree with what I have said. If you decide to take part in the telephone interview today, you will be asked questions about your health during and after your pregnancy, your labor and delivery, and your baby’s health and personality. You will be sent a gift for your baby after the interview as a token of our thanks for participating. All information that you provide will be kept strictly confidential. Your name will not be put on any questionnaires; they will only have an identification number on them. All questionnaires will be kept in locked file cabinets in a locked oflice. Your identity will not be revealed in any reports written about this study. We will summarize information fiom all study participants and will not report inforrmtion about individuals. The only exception is if you indicate that child abuse is occurring in your household; then we are required to notify Protective Services. You have the right to withdraw fi'om this study at any point timing the interview with no penalty or negative consequences. Your decision about whether to participate or not will not affect your relationship with any agencies or with Michigan State University. If you have any questions, please ask us. If you have questions later on, you can contact Dr. Anne Bogat or Dr. Alytia Levendosky at (517) 432-1447. In addition, we would also like to contact you when your baby is 11-12 months old. Your participation today does not obligate you to participate in the third interview. Do I have your permission to continue with the interview? (Circle one) YES NO 134 APPENDIX D Tracking Letter and Information Sheet Date: Dear Thank you again for participating in the Mother-Infant Study at Michigan State University. We really appreciate your willingness to help us learn about the unique and important experiences of mothers and infants in our community. \Vrth the knowledge that we gain from this study, we will be able to develop new and better programs for families like yours. Therefore, your participation in our study will directly benefit many other women and children! We are writing this letter to update our records so that we will be able to reach you about 1 year alter the birth of your baby, at which time we would like to meet with you and your child. You will be paid $75.00 and given a gift for your child at that interview. Please take a moment to fill out the Subject Information Sheet and send it back to us in the enclosed self-addressed, stamped envelope. If you prefer, you may also call us at 432-1447 or 432-7726 to give us your updated information. Please send back the information sheet or call us even if you still have the same address and phone number. As usual, all information will be kept strictly confidential. Thank you again for your help on this important project. Sincerely, Mother-Infant Study Stafl‘ Member 135 Subject Information Sheet Name: Address: (Street Number and Street Name) (City, State and Zip Code) Telephone Number: Alternate Telephone Number: Cell Phone Number: firgeror Beeper Number: E-mail: Other Contact Information: Thank You! ! !! For Ofice Use Only Date Received: 90 day follow-up: 1St or 2"‘1 Other Notes: 136 APPENDIX E Time 3 Consent Form MOTHER-INFANT STUDY CONSENT F ORM-T3 Thank you for participating in the first two interviews for this study. This study is part of a survey of women in Michigan, some of whom may be experiencing domestic violence. We hope to learn about the strengths that you bring to your situation, your feelings, your perceptions of your baby, and your relationships with others, including family members, partners, and friends. We hOpe to use this information to help plan better prograrrrs for families experiencing domestic violence. If you decide to take part in the survey today, you will be asked questions about how you have been feeling recently, events that have lmppened to you in the last year, and your feelings about your baby and the people in your life who provide support for you. You will also be asked to participate in some play with your baby that will be videotaped. Your baby will also play with some toys and games with an interviewer for about an hour. The total interview will take about 3—4 hours. You will be paid $75 for your participation and you will also receive a toy for your baby. All information that you give us will be kept strictly confidential among the project staff. Your name or your baby’s will not be on any questionnaires and videotapes; an identification number will be put on them instead. All questionnaires and videotapes will be kept in locked file cabinets in a locked office. Your identity will not be revealed in any reports written about this study. We will summarize information fi'om all study participants and will not report information about yourself or any individuals. Your privacy will be protected to the maximum extent allowable by law. The only exception to full confidentiality is in the case of ongoing child abuse or neglect. If you indicate that child abuse or neglect is occurring in your household, we are required to make a report to Child Protective Services. We would inform you if we thought we needed to make such a report. You have the right to refuse to answer any questions or to withdraw from this study at any point during the interview with no penalty or negative consequences. Your decision about whether to participate or not will not affect your relationship with any agencies or Michigan State University. If you have any questions, please ask us. If you have any questions about the study later, you can contact Dr. Anne Bogat or Dr. Alytia Levendosky at (517)432-1447. If you have questions about your rights as a participant in this research study you may contact Dr. David Wright at 355-2180. We may be interested in recontacting you 2 years after the birth of your baby. At the end of the interview today, we will ask you to update the contact information that we have for you. Your participation today does not obligate you to participate in any future interviews. l have read this form and agree to participate. Signature of Participant: Date: Witness: Date: Anne Bogat, Ph.D. Alytia Levendosky, Ph.D. Michigan State University Michigan State University Department of Psychology Department of Psychology East Lansing, MI 48824 East Lansing, MI 48824 137 APPENDIX F Perceptions of Adult Attachment Questionnaire PAAQ The majority of the following statements refer to your early childhood relationship with your mother (when you were approximately 3 to 8 years old). If someone else was the principal person responsible for your care in childhood, please respond to the questions which refer to “mother” with that person in mind. A few of the questions have two parts. For example “when I caused trouble as a child I knew my mother would forgive me.” Some people might feel like they never caused trouble as a child; however, they consider their mothers very forgiving. How then do you answer? Only answer AGREE or STRONGLY AGREE if you agree with both parts of the statement. If you agree with only one part of the statement answer NEUTRAL. If you disagree with both parts of the statement answer DISAGREE or STRONGLY DISAGREE. l= STRONGLY DISAGREE 2 = DISAGREE 3 = NEUTRAL (NEITHER DISAGREE NOR AGREE) 4 = AGREE 5 = STRONGLY AGREE 1. In childhood I felt like I was really treasured by my mother. 2. In childhood I sometimes felt like my mother was really lonely when I was not with her. 3. My mother was not very affectionate. 4. .When I was a young child and little things went wrong, 1 did not feel sure I could count on my mother to take care of me. 5. As a child I couldn’t stand being separated fiom my mother. 6. My mother can make me feel really good, but when she is not nice to me she can really tear me apart. 7. In my family of origin we don’t make a show of expressing our feelings. We prefer keeping feelings to ourselves. 8. Neither my mother nor myselfare perfect, but somehow we made it through my childhood. 138 1= STRONGLY DISAGREE 2 = DISAGREE 3 = NEUTRAL (NEITHER DISAGREE NOR AGREE) 4 = AGREE 5 = STRONGLY AGREE 9. I remember, when I was fi'ightened as a child, my mother holding me close. 10. When I was a child, my mother sometimes told me that if I was not good she would stop loving me. 11. My mother is selfishly caught up in herself to the exclusion of everybody else. 12. My family was not particularly intimate, but this has never bothered me. 13. It’s hard for me to remember my early relationship with my mother in any detail. 14. In childhood I sometimes felt that my mother and I were so alike that I didn’t know where she ended and I began. 15. If anything happened to my mother I wonder if I could survive it. 16. I remember as a child feeling a desire to protect my mother. 17. Even though I went through rough times with my mother during my childhood, somewhere along the line I managed to let go of the majority of those angry, hurt feelings. 18. In childhood I knew I was low on my mother’s priority list. 19. My mother was an all-around excellent mother. 20. No one gets under my skin like my mother. 21. As a child I never thought separations from my parents were any big deal. 22. I often felt responsible for my mother’s welfare. 23. In childhood my mother sometimes threatened to leave me or to send me away if I wasn’t good. 24. To this day my mother has no clue who I am or what 1 am all about. 139 l= STRONGLY DISAGREE 2 = DISAGREE 3 = NEUTRAL (NEITHER DISAGREE NOR AGREE) 4 = AGREE 5 = STRONGLY AGREE 25. 26. 27. 28. 29. 30. 31. 32. 33. 34. 35. 36. 37. 38. 39. Even with all our past difficulties, I realize my mother did the best for me that she could. I have forgotten what most of my early childhood was like. I always knew my mother was there for me; no matter what I could depend on her. There are times when I feel like shaking my mother and saying “wake up and see me for who I am.” In childhood I often had the impression that my mother was not listening to me. She ofien tuned me out. During my childhood I sometimes felt like I was my mother’s whole life. My mother and I are more accepting of each other’s differences than we have been in the past. When 1 was young, I ofien feared something dreadful would happen to my mother or father. I remember my mother telling me that I didn’t pay enough attention to her or love her enough. I often take my mother’s opinions about rrre to heart and lose sight of my own opinions about myself. My mother is a real nag. My mother and I were so alike we often could finish each other’s sentences. I think people put too much emphasis on the mother/child relationship. I remember very little about my early childhood (ages three to seven). The concept of the loving, supporting mother is pure myth 140 l= STRONGLY DISAGREE 2 = DISAGREE 3 = NEUTRAL (NEITHER DISAGREE NOR AGREE) 4 = AGREE 5 = STRONGLY AGREE 40. 41. 42. 43. 45. 46. 47. 48. 49. 50. 51. 52. 53. My relationship with my mother has gone through major changes over the course of my childhood and adolescence. Even as an adult I sometimes feel like I will never dig myself out from under my mother’s influence. As a child I sometimes got the feeling that without me my mother would have fallen apart. I couldn’t have asked for a better mother. Ifmymotherwasnotfairtomeasachildlrealizenowitwasbecause she was dealing with her own problems. If something really had happened to me in childhood, I did not feel I could count on my mother to support me. When I was a child I sometimes got the feeling that my mother wished I was never born. I remember when I was a child feeling scared that one or both of my parents would die unexpectedly. My mother can devastate me with her criticisms. In childhood my mother often told me she was sacrificing herself for me. 1 don’t think my early childhood relationship with my mother has any significant influence on who I am today or my present relationships. My mother was always there for me when I needed her. When I acted bad as a child my mother would, at times, threaten to send are away. I never felt like my mother gave me enough attention. 141 l= STRONGLY DISAGREE 2 = DISAGREE 3 = NEUTRAL (NEITHER DISAGREE NOR AGREE) 4 = AGREE 5 = STRONGLY AGREE 54. For all our past problems, my mother and I can still enjoy a good laugh together. 55. During my childhood, my mother would often turn to me and tell me lots of things that upset and bothered me. 56. In childhood, I often worried about my mother’s state of health 57. I find it difficult to remember my early childhood. 58. My mother was a perfect mother. 59. My mother’s issues are still interfering with my life. m 60. When I think back to my early childhood experiences, I discover things about myself and my parents that I’ve never considered before. 142 APPENDIX G Working Model of the Child Interview WMCI INTERVIEWER: PLEASE TURN ON THE TAPE RECORDER AND MCROPHONE. Test the tape recorder by saying “Testing 1, 2 , 3 ” a few times. Rewind and play back the recording to make sure everything is working properly. THEN, TURN ON TAPE REC ORDER-PRESS RECORD—AGAIN. WAIT 5 SECONDS. SAY YOUR NM, DATE, and SUBJECT NUWER INTO THE RECORDER BEGIN MERVIEW. 1. MAKE SURE TAPE RECORDER IS TURNED ON and THAT “RECORD” IS PRESSED. 2. MAKE SURE MICROPHONE IS TURNED ON. We are interested in how parents think and feel about their children before they are born. This interview is a way for us to ask you about that. The interview will take us about an hour to complete. 1a. Let’s start with your pregnancy. I’m interested in things like whether it was planned or unplanned, how you feel physically and emotionally, and what you are doing during the pregnancy (e.g., working). Let’s take these one at a time. [The idea is to put the participant at ease and to begin to obtain a chronological history of the pregnancy. Additional probes may be necessary to make sure that the individual is given a reasonable opportunity to convey the history of their reactions and feelings about the pregnancy and the baby (which may or may not be the same). ] Was the pregnancy planned or unplanned? How much is the baby wanted or not wanted? When did the pregnancy seem real to you? How have you felt physically and emotionally throughout your pregnancy? [ Interviewer: Find out the history of these throughout the pregnancy. ] What are you doing, or have you been doing, during the pregmncy? (e.g., working?) [Interviewer: Find out the history of these throughout the pregnancy. ] What have been your impressions about the baby while you’re pregnant? What do you sense the baby might be like? 143 u" MAKE SURE THE TAPERECORDER IS TURNED ON and “RECORD” IS PRESSED. *m MAKE SURE THE MICROPHONE IS TURNED ON. 1b. How do you think you will react to labor and delivery? What do you think your feelings about labor and delivery will be? What do you think your first reaction will be when you see the baby? What will be your reaction ifthe baby is a boy? Ifthe baby is a girl? How do you think your family will react to the birth of your baby? [Interviewer: Be sure to include husband/partner, other siblings. ] 1c. Do you think your baby will have any problems in the first few days after birth? How long do you think the baby will have to stay in the hospital? Are you going to breast-feed or bottle-feed? Why? How did you come to that decision? 1d. How do you think the first few weeks at home with the baby will go? [Interviewers Explore feelings about feeding, sleeping, crying, etc. ] 1e. How old do you think your baby will be when he/she sits up? Crawls? Walks? Smiles? Talks? Do you think your baby will do these things ahead, behind, or at the same time as other babies? 144 Do you have any sense yet of what your baby’s intelligence will be? Why do you think that? If. Do you think your baby will have a regular routine? What do you think will happen if you or your baby can’t stay in the routine? 1 g. Will you need to be separated from your baby after he/she is born? (e.g., work) If the participant says YES or NO, the interviewer asks: "What do you think this will be like for you? For the baby? If the participant says “I HOPE NOT” the interviewer asks: "If this did happen, what would this be like for you? For the baby? Will there be any separations in the first year of your baby’s life that will last for more than a day? How will that be for you? For your baby? 2a. What do you think your child’s personality will be like when he/she is born? [Personality-the qualities/traits/features that give someone their identity, that makes someone who they are ] 2b. Pick 5 words (adjectives) that describe what your child’s personality will be like when he/ she is born. [ Interviewer: Write these down on the paper for reference. It is not important that participants come up with exactly 5 adjectives] 1. 2 3. 4. 5 For each one, what makes you say that? 3a. Who do you think your baby will be most like? What personality traits do you think your child will inherit fi'om you? What traits will your child inherit from the baby’s father? 145 3b. Do you think there are any characteristics your child will inherit from your side of the family? From the baby’s Ether’s side of the family? 3c. Have you decided on your child’s name? How did you decide?(or How will you decide?) Does that name have special meaning in your Emily or the baby’s Ether’s Emily? 4. In what ways do you think your child will be unique or different fiom other children? 5. After your baby is born, what behavior in his/her first year oflife do you think will be the most dificult for you to handle? Can you give an example? 5a. Why will this be difficult? How often do you think it will occur? What will you feel like doing when your child behaves like that? How will you feel if your child acts this way? What will you do about the behavior? 5b. Do you think your child will know you don’t like that behavior? Why do you think he/she will act like that? So. What do you imagine will happen to this behavior as your child grows older? Why do you think so? 6a. How would you describe your relationship with your baby now, while you’re pregnant? 146 6b. Pick five words (adjectives) to describe your relationship. For each word, describe an incident or memory that illustrates what you mean. [ Interviewer: Write these down on the paper for reference. It is not important that participants come up with exactly 5 adjectives] P‘PP’N?‘ 7a. What pleases you most about your relationship with your baby while you’re pregnant? What do you wish you could change about it? 7b. How do you feel your relationship with your baby while you’re pregnant will affect your baby’s personality? 7c. Has your relationship with your baby changed during the pregnancy? In what ways? What is your feeling about the change? 8. When your baby is born, what parent do you think he/she will be closest to? Why? Do you expect that to change (as the child gets older, for instance)? How do you expect it to change? 9. Do you think your baby will get upset often in his/her first 12 months? What will you do at those times? What do you think your feelings will be at those times? 9a. What about when the baby becomes emotionally upset? What will you do at those times? What do you think your feelings will be at those times? 9b. What about when your child becomes physically hurt a little bit (e.g., hitting his head against the crib)? What will you do at those times? What do you think your feelings will be at those times? 147 9c. What about when your child becomes sick (e.g., he/she gets a fever)? What will you do at those times? What do you think your feelings will be at those times? 10. Tell me a favorite story about your pregnancy, perhaps one you’ve told to Emily or fiiends. I’ll give you a minute to think about this one. [ Interviewer: If the participant is struggling, you may tell them that this doesn ’t have to be the favorite story, only a favorite one. ] What do you like about this story? 11a. Can you think of any experiences you’ve had during your pregnancy that might have been a setback for your baby? Why do you think so? |Setbac =something that happened that makes things harder for your baby than for other babies.] If person says ITS, then ask: ”"Why do you think so? If person says NO, go to next question. [ Interviewer: Indirectly, we ’re trying to determine whether the parent feels responsible in any way for the setbacks. ] Knowing what you know now, if you started all over again with your pregnancy, what would you do differently? 1 lb. Are there any experiences your baby might have during the first year of his/her life that might be a setback for him/her? If person says NO, go to question 12. If person says ITS, then ask: Why do you think so? Who or what is likely to contribute to these setbacks? Is there anything you might do to prevent these setbacks? 12. Do you ever worry about your unborn baby? What do you worry about? 13. If your child could be any age right now (unborn, 1 month, 1 year, etc.), what age would you choose? Why? 148 14. As you look ahead, what will be the most difficult time in your child’s development? Why do you think so? 15. What do you expect your child to be like as an adolescent? Wlmt makes you feel this way? What do you expect to be good and not so good about this period in your child’s life? 16. Think for a moment of your child as an adult. What hopes and fears do you have about that time? INTER VIEWER: I. TURN TAPE RECORDER AND MICROPHONE OFF. 2. GIVE PART ICIPANT THE INT ER VIEW PACKET. 149 APPENDIX H Maternal Efficacy Questionnaire MEQ We want to ask you some questions about yourself and your baby. We are trying to get a general idea of how you usually handle different situations with your baby. We realize tlnrt no one is always effective or always ineffective. We all do better in some situations than in others. So we would like to have you think about some situations that all mothers encounter. 1. When your baby is upset, fussy or crying, how good are you at soothing him or her? 1 2 3 4 not good at all not good enough good enough very good 2. How good are you at understanding what your baby wants or need? For example, do you know whenyourbabyneedstobechangedorwantstobefed? l 2 3 4 not good at all not good enough good enough very good 3. How good are you at making your baby understand what you want him/her to do? For example, if you want your baby to eat dinner or play quietly, how good are you at making him or her do that? l 2 3 4 not good at all not good enough good enough very good 4. How good are you at getting your baby to pay attention to you? For example, when you want your baby to look at you, how good are you at making him or her do it? 1 2 3 4 not good at all not good enough good enough very good 5. How good are you at getting your baby to have fun with you? For example, how good are you at getting your baby to smile and laugh with you? 1 2 3 4 not good at all not good enough good enough very good 6. How good are you at knowing what activities your baby will enjoy? For example, how good are you at knowing what games and toys your baby will like to play with? l 2 3 4 not good at all not good enough good enough very good 150 7. How good are you at keeping your baby occupied when you need to do housework? For example, how good are you at finding things for the baby when you need to do the dishes. 1 2 3 4 not good at all not good enough good enough very good 8. How good do you feel you are at feeding, changing, and bathing your baby? 1 2 3 4 not good at all not good enough good enough very good 9. How good are you at getting your baby to show off for visitors? For example, how good are you at making your baby smile or laugh for people who visit? 1 2 3 4 not good at all not good enough good enough very good 10. In general, how good a mother do you feel you are? l 2 3 4 not good at all not good enough good enough very good 151 APPENDIX I The Strange Situation (SS): Laboratory Procedures Episode 1 (30 seconds): The mother and baby are introduced to the experimental room by experimenter. Mother is shown where to set the baby down and where to sit. The examiner leaves the room as soon as she has completed the instructions. Episode 2 (3 minutes): The mother and baby are alone in the experimental room. The mother sits in her chair and pretends to read a magazine. The mother has been instructed ahead of time that she should not initiate conversation with the baby, although she can respond to baby’s initiation of interaction. If after 2 minutes the baby has not explored the toys, the mother is signaled (through a knock on the wall by the experimenter) to help the baby become interested in the toys. Episode 3 (3 minutes): A stranger (who has never met the baby before) enters the room and introduces herself to the mother. The stranger immediately sits in her designated chair and remains silent for 1 minute. At the end of 1 minute, the stranger begins a conversation with the mother. At the end of another minute, the stranger is signaled (by experimenter) to initiate interaction with the baby. At the end of 3 minutes total, the mother unobtrusively leaves the room, leaving her purse or handbag behind on her chair. Episode 4 (3 minutes): As soon as the mother leaves, the stranger reduces interaction with the baby so that the baby can notice that the mother is gone. Stranger returns to her chair. If the baby cries, the stranger will try to distract the baby with toys. If necessary, the stranger will attempt to calm the baby by talking to or picking up him/her. If baby is calmed, the stranger puts him/her down and attempts to engage the baby with the toys. Episode 5 (3 minutes): The mother approaches the closed door and speaks outside, loudly enough that the baby can hear her. She pauses, opens the door, and pauses again to let the baby respond. Meanwhile, the stranger leaves unobtrusively. After 3 minutes, the mother leaves the room She pauses at the door and says “bye-bye,” closing the door behind her. Episode 6 (3 minutes): The baby is left alone to explore the room for 3 minutes. If the baby becomes acutely distressed, this episode is terminated, but the remaining time is added onto episode seven. Episode 7 (3 minutes): The stranger approaches the door and speaks outside, loudly enough that the baby can hear her voice. She pauses, opens the door, and pauses again allowing the baby to respond. If the baby is crying, the stranger will attempt to soothe the baby. Whenthebabyiscahnthe strangerwillgraduallyretumtoherchair. Episode 8 (3 minutes): The mother opens the door and pauses for a moment, giving the babyachanceto respond. Themotherthentalksto the babyandpickshim/herup. The stranger leaves the room. 152 ANfiéfidfida 3:20:23. v—mOBHmZ éZOmmmm 22%. cc ceaz Ea 4Nmeaesmm d:— Som E 38.—on 58595 26; so» mm 80an >55 me 9.: 38% em .5 8: 2 26: 5: ea 90> 55o: EEEEEEEE: E355 .5 5.858.- m53>oa 28 550:: £356.... 85583 .528 .5 3.53... 3.55.. 35 5505—52» .558 .5505 $28.5 835.2 .5 £382: 388.. 5g .5 095%.. .88 So» 5 been me 282— %:aE me «a: new :5» B Edges: ciao...— ofi he gem... .5.» Eu: 2 a: megs—em 05 83 Acids-fur; essence. 253 e ceaz ram "oi—Seam ”03555 mess—.8 05 5 ma ding—25.2 05 3865 .55 use £535 .5 8.5: new bee um: 45» 2 38.—8.5 2a 2.3 .5 .5.» ..e toga—8 3:82 0235 0:3 28.—on o5 =e 52300 .29. 2.: .5 0.:— Sox 5 cannon ESE—Em :25 a: 88E OEHM6: 3. 5:250 "m 5:250 "a 55830 3 5252.0 Egg—ware .Seanzwmnv .2 a see u m an a 2:... n m 20555:. n N 205.555 u N 2:: a u _ 0.5: a u _ 52552212 zgmeoeuo 5:95 55 552:: 05 5 west», .3 52525 wage—.5 2: 5325 88:. .35: so» :85: :25 .5: 154 .vn .vm .8 .3 .NN .NN .3 ..N .3 .8 .a— d— .E .m— .: .2 .5. .5— .m _ .m _ .3 .3 .m _ .m— .N_ .m— .: .2 .o. .5— .o d .m .w .2. K .5 .5 .m .n .5 .v .m .m .N .N ._ ._ A585 05 2 .5» 8.8 :23 .525: 25 :5: 22.—3:05 w:_.55 5098 2 5:3 .55 :23 5w .5: 55 552.205 53: 2.58.5: 25 :5: 5w no» 55 .65:qu 8522222 58585 :02: Bo: :52: 85552 5:: 835: 22:: so: “a 5:850 "2. 5.52.0 =55 59m 5 n v .5 a as: n M 22:555.: .1. m 2:: a u 2 .5: 5 5: u c .3 .MN .NN .NN ._N 3 .cm .3 .3 .2 .2 .m— .: .2.— o. .5— .n _ .n _ .3 .3 m. .m— .N_ .N. .3 . : c. d— .o d .w .w .2. .2. .5 .5 .w .n .5 .v .m .m .N .N ._ .— 2222 2.2.2. :85: 25 5.8 2.3% 20: :85: 25 5.38 :85 Bo: .20: 325222 55: :02: Bo: .383 358m 5.: 2:8. 5 .585 05 2 05: a 5B 2 505:8 053 25%.: .55 385: 8 5058: .52.: 5 5.52.0 “m 838:0 .85 50..» a n v 2: a 02:: n n 2055558 u N 2:: a n _ =2: 5 5: u o 155 .mm .mm .- .2 ..N ..N .on .2... .2 .2 .2 .3 .2 .2 .2 e. .2 m _ .3 .1 .2 .2 .2 .2 .2 : .o- c— .a .o .w .m .N. H .e .e .m .m .v .v .m .m .n .m ._ ._ m8. 8 .8» a 8:0 u _ 38» m 55 0.58 n m gamoEmHBomanm Eogmoamuv .258... u m 38» m 2 _ n m 583 n v £85 2 2 o u N 33. u m mficoE o .85 m8— " _ A3032 6 53> .mzao 25:3 28.2. £5 :23 83:8 9:5 5.8me $5 .233: :9» av bacon—xx“ So: 8505— 33 96; 32 30$ fi— 538:0 u: 533:0 em .vm mm .MN 2 an _N .3 .2 .3 d— d— .2 .2 .2 .2 2 .2 n _ .n _ 3 .3 .2 .2 :2 .m— ._ _ .2 d— .c. .o .o .w w .h N. .e o .m m .9 v .m m .N N ._ _ A353 a $5» 23.38:. 8 JOEE... 288 .08: 2.3% 5:3 .6» 2 38m: ..wdv .5 £33.55 Soon .5 .oNBES $.8th £5 :5: 6w .5» cu .65,—wow; com—on £5 moon :02: 3o: Sena tang—a 35:25 :02: 3o: 5— 328:0 “a 3.3.2.0 . 3% 82m a n v E a 3:... u m 338038 n m 2:: a u _ =m 3 8: n o 156 .9... .85 < .v .5 a 230 .m 55:5 055.55 < .N 2:: < ._ :6 a 0:62 .6 «so» 2 2:253: 5mg. 9.. 95 0:3 0:58 08:: .3 3355 .53 con—95 =5» .5 :02: 3c: £955 :2 $325 .25 _::a.Smo_5\§m_£E 3530:: 5 5.3550 502.65 28 :28: 53:30: 05583 50:8 5 :53 manor: Steam 835.2 5 95:52: 38:: 55:5 .5 3.5% 45.: 2 2:255 5mg. o: 98 0:3 x5350 :28 :5: £558 .5 5:53: 05 286£ 887. am— nm hm n: mm> .— 335 38.35:: 538:.» a: QZ§ ...53m.~382t OZ .o 3:88: 5:5 2:8 5 .58: 60:838. 5 0053: 655:0 :o_. a $555 3 26 main—5:22 agtoas: .95 5o. .5.» 96: .56.: 5.5 0:: :35 .m— 157 APPENDIX K Postnatal Pregnancy Support Items 1. How much advice and guidance about parenting do you get from this person (e. g., baby’s diet, how to comfort the baby)? 2. How much practical assistance about parenting do you get from this person (e. g., take you to the pediatrician, lend or give you things for the baby, babysit)? 3. How much emotional support about parenting do you get from this person (e. g., listens to you when you are upset or worried)? 158 7. APPENDIX L Demographic Questionnaire Yourdateofbirth: __ /__ /__ (m0) (613') (yr) Your baby’s due date: / / Eric) (21;) (36 Have you been pregnant before? (Circle one) 1 = YES 2 = NO (If NO, go to Question 7) If YES, to Question 3: 4. How many times? 5. Have you had any miscarriages, still births, or abortions? (Circle one) 1 = YES 2 = NO 6. How many biological children do you currently have? How many people, including yourself, live in your household? (If participant is living in a shelter, questions 7 & 8 refer to household composition before moving into shelter.) 8. Please list these: (Write in specific relationship to mother. Be specific--is the person (for ex.) a husband, stepfather, biological child, foster child, or partner's child?) 9. Choose the one that best describes your current marital/relationship status (choose only one): (a) single, never married (see below) (b) married For how long? (in months) (c) separated For how long? (in months) (d) divorced For how long? (in months) (e) widowed For how long? (in months) If (a) is circled: Are you currently in a relationship? YES NO If YES, go to Question 10. If NO, were you in a relationship that lasted at least 6 weeks during your current pregnancy? YES NO 10. First name of your current partner or the partner you were with for at least 6 159 11. 12. 13. 14. 15. 16. weeks during your pregnancy: Are you currently living with your partner/spouse? (Circle one) 1 = YES 2 = NO If yes to Question 11, how long have you been doing so? (Circle one) 1=lessthan 1 year 2 =1-3 years 3 =4-6 years 4= 7-9 years 5 = 10-12 years 6= 13-15 years 7 =16 - 18 years 8 =19 - 21 years 9=22 -24 years 10 = 25 or more years Prior to your current romantic relationship, specified in Question #10 (a) were you ever married? 1 = YES 2 = NO (b) did you ever live with a partner? 1 = YES 2 = NO (c) were you ever separated? 1 = YES 2 = NO ((1) were you ever divorced? 1 = YES 2 = NO (e) were you ever widowed? l = YES 2 = NO What is your current relationship with the father of your baby? (Circle one) 1 = spouse 2 = ex-spouse 3 = partner 4 = ex-partner 5 = friend 6 = acquaintance 7 = stranger 8 = other Please specify: What is your racial or ethnic group? (Circle one) 1 = Native American 2 = Asian American/Pacific Islander 3 = Black, African American 4 = Latino, Hispanic, Chicano 5 = Biracial (mixed): Specify 6 = Caucasian, White 7 = Other: What is the baby’s father’s racial or ethnic group? (Circle one) 1 = Native American 160 2 = Asian American/Pacific Islander 3 = Black, African American 4 = Latino, Hispanic, Chicano 5 = Biracial (mixed): Specify 6 = Caucasian, White 7 = Other: 17. What is the highest level of education you have completed? (Circle one) 1 = grades 1, 2, 3, 4, 5, or 6 (circle specific grade) 2 = grades 7, 8, 9, 10, 11, 12, GED (circle specific grade) 3 = some college Where? 4 = AA degree Where? 5 = BA/BS Where? 6 = some grad school Where? 7 = graduate degree Where? MA? Ph.D.? Law? MD? 8 = other; Specify (e.g., Beauty School, nursing school) 18. Do you currently work outside the home? YES NO If N0, did you work outside the home during the last year? YES NO 19. If YES to either part of Question 18, what is/was your occupation? Please be specific. For example, bookkeeper, cashier, computer programmer. If there were two jobs/occupations, have participant choose the one that she feels best represents her occupation. 20. What is the highest level of education your partner/spouse has completed? (Circle one) 1 = grades 1, 2, 3, 4, 5, or 6 (circle specific grade) 2 = grades 7, 8, 9, 10, 11, 12, GED (circle specific grade) 3 = some college Where? 4 = AA degree Where? 5 = BA/BS Where? 6 = some grad school Where? 7 = graduate degree Where? MA? Ph.D.? Law? MD? 161 8 = other; Specify (e.g., Beauty School, nursing school) 21. Does s/he work outside the home? (Circle one) 1 = YES 2 = NO 22. If yes to Question 20, what is his/her occupation? 23. What is your total family income per month (estimate)? 24. Do you currently receive any public assistance? (Circle one) 1 = YES 2 = NO 25. Are you currently residing in a shelter for battered women? YES NO # days? 26. Have you ever stayed in a shelter for battered women before . . . (a) Because of your experience of abuse? YES NO # days? (b) Because of your mother’s/guardian’s experience of abuse? YES NO # days? 27. Have you ever stayed in a homeless shelter before . . . (a) Because of your experiences? YES NO # days? (b) Because of your parents’lguardians’ 162 APPENDIX M Beck Depression Inventory BDI In answering these questions, think about each item carefully and circle the answer out of the group of 4 items that best reflects how you have been feeling during the past week. 1. [1] [2] [3] [4] [1] [2] [3] [4] [1] [2] [3] [4] [1] [2] [3] [4] [1] [2] [3] [4] [1] [2] [3] [4] [1] [2] [3] [4] [1] [2] [3] [4] I do not feel sad. I feel sad. Iamsadallthetimeandlcan’t snapoutofit. Iamso sadorunhappythatlcan’tstandit. I am not particularly discouraged about the future. I feel discouraged about the future. I feel I have nothing to look forward to. I feel that the future is hopeless and that things cannot improve. I do not feel like a failure. I feel I have failed more than the average person. As I look back on my life, all I can see is a lot offailures. I feel I am a complete failure as a person. I get as much satisfaction out of things as I used to. I don’t enjoy things the way I used to. I don’t get real satisfaction out of anything anymore. I am dissatisfied or bored with everything. I don’t feel particularly guilty. I feel guilty a good part of the time. I feel quite guilty most of the time. I feel guilty all of the time. I don’t feel I am being punished. I feel I may be punished. I expect to be punished. I feel I am being punished. I don’t feel disappointed in myself. I am disappointed in myself. I am disgusted with myself. I hate myself. I don’t feel I am any worse than anybody else. I am critical of myself for all my weaknesses or mistakes. I blame myselfall the time for my faults. I blame myself for everything bad that happens. 163 10. 11. 12. 13. 14. 15. 16. 17. [1] [2] [3] [4] [1] [2] [3] [4] [1] [2] [3] [4] [1] [2] [3] [4] [1] [2] [3] [4] [1] [2] [3] [4] [1] [2] [3] [4] [1] [2] [3] [4] [1] [2] [3] [4] I don’t have any thoughts of killing myself. I have thoughts of killing myself, but I would not carry them out. I would like to kill myself. Iwouldkillmyselfiflhadthechance. Idon’t cryany morethanusual. Icrymorenowthanlusedto. Icryallthetirnenow. Iusedto beable to cry, but nowI can’t cryeven thoughl want to. Iamno more irritatedbythingsthanleveram. I am slightly more irritated now than usual I am quite annoyed or irritated a good deal of the time. I feelirritatedallthetimenow. I have not lost interest in other people. I am less interested in other people than I used to be. I have lost most of my interest in other people. I have lost all of my interest in other people. I make decisions about as well as I ever could. I put ofl‘ making decisions more than I used to. I have greater difficulty in making decisions than before. I can’t make decisions at all anymore. I don’t feel that I look any worse than I used to. I am worried that I am looking old or unattractive. I feel that there are permanent changes in my appearance that make me look unattractive. I believe that I look ugly. I can work about as well as before. It takes an extra effort to get started at doing something. I have to push myself very hard to do anything. I can’t do any work at all. I can sleep as well as usual. I don’t sleep as well as I used to. I wake up 1-2 hours earlier than usual and find it hard to get back to sleep. I wake up several hours earlier than I used to and cannot get back to sleep. I don’t get more tired than usual. I gettired more easilythanlusedto. I get tired fi'om doing almost everything. I amtoo tired to do anything. 164 18. 19. 20. 21. [1] [2] [3] [4] My appetite is no worse than usual. My appetite is not as good as it used to be. My appetite is much worse now. I have no appetite at all anymore. I haven’t lost much weight, if any, lately. I have lost more than five pounds. I have lost more than ten pounds. I have lost more than fifteen pounds. I am no more worried about my health than usual. I am worried about physical problems such as aches and pains, or upset stomach, or constipation. I am very worried about my physical problems and it’s hard to think of much else. I am so worried about my physical problems that I cannot think about anything else. I have not noticed any recent change in my interest in sex. I am less interested in sex thanI used to be. I am much less interested in sex now. I have lost interest in sex completely. 165 APPENDIX N Severity of Violence Against Women Scales VAW Scales-Pregnancy Interview, Pt. I [NAME see Page 2, Question 10].***** You and have probably experienced anger or conflict. Below is a list of behaviors he may have done. Describe how often he has done each behavior at 2 different times (during your current pregnancy and the year before you became pregnant) by choosing a letter fi'om the following scale. [Interviewer: (f “*“This questionnaire refers to Acted like he wanted to kill you Held you down, pinning you in place _Pushed or shoved you _Shook or roughly handled you 166 Physically forced you to have sex Made you have anal sex against your will Usedanobjectonyouinasexual way you suddenly or forcefully participant was not involved in a relationship with dwing the year before she became pregnant, code E] A BB C D E never once a few times many times not applicable During your current pregnancy: During your current pregnancy: The year before you became pregnant: The year before you became pregnant: _Hit, kicked a wall, door, or furniture _ _Scratched you __ _Threw, smashed or broke an object _ _Pulled your hair __ _Driven dangerously with you in the _ _Twisted your arm car _ _Spanked you _ _Threw an object at you __ __Bit you __ _Shook a finger at you _ _Slapped you with the palm of hand __ _Made threatening gestures or faces at _ _Slapped you with the back of hand you __ _Slapped you around your lace/head __ _Shook a fist at you __ _Kicked you __ _Acted like a bully toward you __ _Hit you with an object __ _Destroyed something belonging to __ _____Stomped on you you _ _Choked you __ _Threatened to harm or damage things __ _Punched you you care about __ _Btu'ned you with something _ _Threatened to destroy property _ _Used 3 club-like object on you __ _Threatened someone you care about _ _Beat you up __ _Threatened to hurt you __ _Used a knife or gun on you _ _Threatened to kill himself _ _Dernanded sex whether you wanted _ _Threatened you with a club-like to or not object _Made you have oral sex against your __ _Threatened you with a knife or gun will __ _Threatened to kill you _ _Made you have sexual intercourse _ _Threatened you with a weapon against your will Severity of Violence Against Women Scales-Previous Partner VAW Scales—Pregnancy Interview, Pt. II How often did your most recent previous partner (the person before (NAME) engage in each of these activities with you? [ Interviewer: Relationship with previous partner must have lasted 6 weeks or longer in order to complete questionnaire. ] Please give the dates of this relationship: / to / (mo) (yr) (mo) (yr) A BB C D E never once a few times many times not applicable How often did your pamter: How ofien did your partner: _Hit, kicked a wall, door, or furniture _ _Scratched you _Threw, smashed or broke an object __ _Pulled your hair _Driven dangerously with you in the _ _Twisted yom arm car _ _Spanked you _ _Threw an object at you _ _Bit you _ _Shook a finger at you __ _Slapped you with the palm of hand _ _Made threatening gestures or faces at _ _Slapped you with the back of hand you _ _Slapped you around your face/head __ _Shook a fist at you __ _Kicked you _ _Acted like a bully toward you _ _Hit you with an object _Destroyed something belonging to __ _Stomped on you you __ _Choked you __ _Threatened to harm or damage things _ _Punched you you care about _ _Burned you with something _ _Threatened to destroy property __ _Used a club-like object on you _ _Threatened someone you care about __ _Beat you up __ _Threatened to hurt you __ _Used a knife or gun on you _ _Threatened to kill himself _ _Demanded sex whether you wanted _ _Threatened you with a club-like to or not object __ _Made you have oral sex against your _ _Threatened you with a knife or gun will _ _Threatened to kill you __ _Made you have sexual intercourse __ _Threatened you with a weapon against your will _ _Acted like he wanted to kill you __ _Physically forced you to have sex __ _Held you down, pinning you in _ _Made you have anal sex against your place will _Pushedorshovedyou __ _ Usedanobjectonyouinasexual __ _Shook or roughly handled you way Grabbed you suddenly or forcefirlly 167 APPENDIX 0 Severity of Violence Against Women Scale-Time 3 Interview You and your partner have probably experienced anger or conflict. Below is a list of behaviors he may have done. Describe how often he has done each behavior during the last year by choosing a letter fi'om the following scale. I Interviewer: If participant was not involved in a relationship with a partner drm'ng the year code E] A BB C D E never once a few times many times not applicable During the last year, how often did your partner: How often did your partner: _ _Hit or kicked a wall, door or furniture __ _Scratched you __ _Hit, kicked a wall, door, or furniture _ _Pulled your hair _ _Threw, smashed or broke an object __ __’l’wisted your arm __ _Driven dangerously with you in the __ _Spanked you car _ _Bit you _ _Threw an object at you _ _Slapped you with the palm of hand __ _Shook a finger at you __ _Slapped you with the back of hand _ _Made threatening gestures or firces at _ _Slapped you around your face/head you __ _Kicked you __ _Shook a fist at you _ _Hit you with an object __ _Acted like a bully toward you _ _Destroyed something belonging to __ _Stomped on you you __ _Choked you __ _Threatened to harm or damage things __ _Punched you you care about __ _Burned you with something _ _Threatened to destroy property _ _Used a club-like object on you __ _Threatened someone you care about __ _Beat you up __ _Threatened to hurt you __ _Used a knife or gun on you __ _Threatened to kill himself __ _Demanded sex whether you wanted _ _Threatened you with a club-like to or not object __ _Made you have oral sex against your _ _Threatened you with a knife or gun will _ _Threatened to kill you _ _Made you have sexual intercourse _ _Threatened you with a weapon against your will __ _Acted like he wanted to kill you __ _Physically forced you to have sex _Held you down, pinning you in _ _Made you have anal sex against your place will __ _Pushedorshovedyou __ __ Usedanobjectonyouinasexual _Shook or roughly handled you way _ _ Grabbed you suddenly or forcefully 168 APPENDIX P Parent Behavior Checklist PBC This checklist includes statements about how parents raise young children. For each statement, mark the number 1 if the statement ALMOST ALWAYS OR ALWAYS applies to how you raise your child. Mark the number 2 if the statement FREQUENTLY applies. Mark the number 3 if the statement SOMETIMES applies. Mark the number 4 if the statement ALMOST NEVER OR NEVER applies. A sample item is shown below. Almost Always=l Frequently=2 Sometimss=3 Almost Never/Never“! 99999.“? 10. l I. 12. 13. 14. 15. l6. 17. 18. 19. 20. 21. 22. 23. 24. 25. 26. 27. 28. 29. 30. I spank my child at least once a week. If my child would hit, kick, bite, or scratch someone, I would spank him/her. If my child hit me in anger, I would hit or spank my child. When my child doesn’t do what I tell him/her to do I spank him/her. I tell my child that his/her bad behavior will make God sad. I yell at my child for whining. I tell my child he/she should be ashamed of him/herself for soiled pants (bowel movement). If my child is overactive, I yell at him/her. If my child cries after being put to bed, I spank him/her. To toilet train my child, I make him/her sit on the toilet for over 15 minutes. I spank my child for refusing to eat. I would spank my child in public for bad behavior. I yell at my child for being too noisy at home. I scold my child for soiling in his/her pants. I threaten to tell my spouse/partner about my child’s bad behavior. I tell my child that he/she is bad. I scold my child for playing with his/her private parts. I tell my child to behave so that my spouse/partner won’t get mad. If my child cries after being put to bed, I yell at him/her. I yell at my child for spilling food. I get so angry with my child I spank him/her on the bottom. I punish my child for wetting the bed. I make my child stay at the table until all of his/her food is gone. I would spank my child for wetting his/her pants. I would slap my child for being sassy or backtalking. I threaten to punish my child but then I don’t. I hit my child with an object (such as a spoon or belt). I tell my child God doesn’t like children who lie. When my child has a temper tantrum, I spank him/her. I send my child to bed as a punishment. 169 rm. . m- . ‘ APPENDIX Q Physical and Emotional Neglect Scale Please answer the following items usinLthe scale below. Never Almost Sometimes Frequently Ahnost Always Never Always l 2 3 4 5 6 _ 1. I hug and kiss my child everyday. __ 2. I feed my child on a regular schedule. __ 3. I have run into the store and left my child in the car for a few minutes. _ 4. During the day, I let my child cry for 15 minutes before I pick him/ha up. __ 5. My child has had diaper rash. __ 6. I play games with my child that will help him/her develop new skills. __ 7. I put prescription and over-the-counter medicine out of the reach of my child. __ 8. I change my child’s diaper 4 times a day or more. __ 9. I take my child to the doctor for regular checkups. __ 10. I talk back and forth with my child every day. __ l 1. Once my child is asleep for the night, I have left him/her alone so that I could go out. _____ 12. I give my child lots of encouragement when he/she does something new (such as walking or making a new sound). __ 13. It is hard to know when my child is hungry. _ 14. I play with my child every day. __ 15. I arrange child care when I can’t be with my child. __ 16. Whatever I do for my child, he/she is never satisfied. _;__ 17. I have left my child at home alone during the daytime so that I could run a quick errand. __ 18. I tell my child that I love him/her every day. __ 19. I sometimes let my child go a couple of days without washing him/her. __ 20. I have trouble making sure my house gets cleaned regularly. __ 21. I try to provide time for my child to play with other children. __ 22. I put child safety plugs on all the electrical outlets in my house. _ 23. If I’m upset with my child, I sometimes don’t talk to him/her all day. __ 24. I change my child’s diaper less than 2 times a day. __ 25. It’s hard to understand what my child is crying about. __ 26. I feed my child within 30 minutes of when s/he is hungry. __ 27. There are days when I’m so busy, that I barely see or talk to my child. 28. I put household cleaning products and chemicals in cabinets that my child can’t get open. __ 29. During the day, I let my child cry for 30 minutes or longer before I pick him/her up. 30. I feed my child at least 3 times a day. 170 vomd Nemd- oomd- dend- eemd- 35d eedd- dm ~ d Nood- mmdd mm _ d mm _ d doc: Q _N _ d NoNd- momd- eeNd- emvd- madd- eo : d- md _ d emdd- mod- N: _ d mm: d weed cod.— 2 edmd mmmd- m _ m d- meNd- deed- mudd- mm— d- am: d deod- deod- md _ d 03 d emod mmed Odd.— d— 3 _ d NmNd- edmd- vmmd- vemd- d: :d- dm _ d- mN : d mmdd- _vod- wood we _ d mood cued dNed odd.— Ne _ d mmmd- dcmd- ddmd- _Nvd- madd- mm _ d- 9; d wedd- eedd- oedd on _ d vmod Nmed awed mmed coo.— emdd ee _ d- ew _ d- emNd- _Nvd- omdd need- em _ d N—dd vmdd- ewdd e_ _d ecod emed Nded wood tamed doc: Edd- N— _d eddd mmNd god 3; d- deod- mo: d- mm — d- dm: d- e_md- me ~ d- dm _ d- wde- gad- mm _ d- mm _ d- momd- Odd; emdd- mm _ d momd mend m— ~d modd- d _dd- m m m d- 3 _d- Vm _ d- mand- m fid- em _ d- 9; d- nond- m m _ d- me _ d- evod- _de doc: add- 3 _ d mood em _ d an _ d mead- md: d- doNd- e— _d- 3 _ d- mdmd- mm _ d- _mmd- mend- dend- dmmd- edmd- demd- _Nvd em _ d odd.— vo _ d- wood comd m ~ Nd em _ d dadd- m 8d- mm _ d- em _ d- dwmd- mwmd- cdmd- mw ~ d- 3 _ d- w _ Nd- :3 d- we— d- 3.— d- mwmd mmmd emmd Odd.— ded m 8d e: _d- Nodd- dmod- d—Nd e: d fwd eVNd memd memd wmmd 3 _d N: _d S _d weed eedd em _ d misd- mmmd- e _ md- owed- coo.— N Neod- em _ d mm _ d d3 d mN— d mde- cm _ d- de _ d- mund- eemd- mmmd- emmd- NmNd- v3 d- ommd- we _ d- _dmd- Na d- mmmd meed ddvd coed mead- doc.— _ em mm mm 3.. on e— m: e— w— m— 3 m: N— _— d— v-‘NMVWOFQO mozaa> 382 325% as: 852:8 M X_Dmem< 171 .m 23 5 Ba: 2.. ”Season 32% assess. sacrum 8302.032 220nm .8835 gaseous geared .2532": .me unannogucm .eoasm 2535 fine .283 .882: 2.": £83 3885 men: .eaaam gee-.5 Sue. eaasm .855 Eu: .eaasm _aaeosm Eu: .8372 Sam .0 an: 623nm. .8§a§< 623". _ .eseeem _ozaue .88sfio 623k 356 2 message 623"” £892»: .3 aosea 623A 5852 oz 05:70 5:585; USE-m .3383 35".. .35.. 05:3 .93 o< .25: En do €32 accustom > X52973. 179 8.88., 8: 32. .e 8... 2.. s 88 m 23. 5 BE. .8 88888 8.885 8583:. 285-8 .28 _ 2st 8885 2858-2 .28 _ 2:5 88.8.89 3882L~ .wa gigofiouom .885 8585 fine 688m .885 2.": .888 3885 flu: 8.35 8885 in: .885 .889: 2.": .885 3885 in: .82-2 gum 5 .8: 623-2 .8csa§< 623-: 53283 623-2 88880 623-8 .885 2 8:88 623-» 88888.. 8 8:82 623A .882 oz 055-» 8:385; 055-“ .3383 055% .35. o<<._um .23 o<§u~ .88? 055-. new 8.88., 882 ooo. _ mm wo~d- ooo.— mm omfi d- b _ md ooo.— _m wmmd- mmwd wood ooo.— om _o _ .o _o _ .o- Rod- owod ooo.— @— oood oood- mood _o _ d wood ooo; w— ww_ d wNNd- momd- mood ohm d own d ooo.— C owod Sod- wm _ d- mwod mmmd Nwmd mwfio ooo.— o_ wmod o3 .o- 2 _d- wmod mwmd omwd ommd mmod ooo.— m— ww ~ d wad- momd- mood owmd mwmd ommd oowd oomd ooo.— w— m—od dood- wand- mmod- momd mo _ d oomd om ~ d wo ~ d mwmd ooo.— n— mm an _N om o— w— 2 o~ 3 w— 2 N— '— '— FNMVWOBMO‘O fi 180 APPENDIX Z Standardized Residual Values for Observed and Latent Variables Observed Variables: Residual Value: 1. Rejection -.02 2. Love .27 3. Angry .32 4. Derogating .85 5. Vulnerability .80 6. No Memory .76 7. Richness of Perceptions .36 8. Openness to Change .26 9. Coherence .32 10. Sensitivity .22 11. Acceptance .23 12. Representation Of Self .38 13. Self-Efficacy .15 14. T1 Emotional Support .52 15. T1 Practical Support .33 16. T1 Caregiving Support .43 17. T3 Emotional Support .56 18. T3 Practical Support .37 19. T3 Caregiving Support .26 20. Demographic Risk .40 21 . Maternal Depression .84 22. Domestic Violence .69 23. Infant Attachment Security .67 Latent Variables: 1. Maternal Representations of 1.00 Attachment 2. Prenatal Representation of Infant .55 3. Prenatal Representation of Self .00 as Mother 4. Postnatal Self-Efficacy .88 5. Prenatal Social Support 1.00 6. Postnatal Social Support .80 7. Risk Factors 1.00 8. Mother-Infant Attachment 1.00 181 APPENDIX AA Lisrel 8.5 Syntax for Final Model Test of original dissertation model with risk revised for T1 da ni=23 no=207 la rejectio love angry derogate vulnerab nomemory rofp optoch coher caresens accept recmse totalmeq tlfemoav t1faidavt1fpreavt3femoavt3faidav t3 fpreav demorisk bdirisk dvrisk overconv rafi=newfirll.dat mo ny=23 ne=8 te=sy,fi ps=sy,fi be=fi,fu le momatt wmci mse meq tlsocsup t3$ocsup risk infatt frtel1te22te33te44te5Ste66te77te88te99te1010 frtell 11te1414te15 15te1616tel717te1818 fi'te 19 19te 20 20te 21 21 te 22 22 frpsl 1p322p855p366ps77 fi'ps33ps44p888te 12 12 valte23 23 te 13 13 frlyllly211y3lly4lly511y61 frly721y821y921y102ly112 frly123 frlyl34 fi'ly1451y1551y165 frlyl76ly186lyl96 frly207ly217ly227 frly238 fibe21be3 1 frbe32 frbe43 fi-be65 frbe46 frbe84 frbe82 fibe27 frpsSlps68p828 fi'te221te201te223te64te53te61te31te225te18 15te222te203te 1422 frte1417te2221te216te1321te919te2021te722te57te723te719te414 frte316te521te321 Pd ou setvscrsmime=mlad=oflit=1500 182 APPENDIX BB Goodness of Fit Statistics Degrees of Freedom = 194 Nomial Theory Weighted Least Squares Chi-Square = 242.03 (P = 0.011) Estimated Non-centrality Parameter (NCP) = 48.03 90 Percent Confidence Interval for NCP = (12.45 ; 91.77) Minimum Fit Function Value = 1.26 Population Discrepancy Function Value (F0) = 0.23 90 Percent Confidence Interval for F0 = (0.060 ; 0.45) Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA) = 0.035 90 Percent Confidence Interval for RMSEA = (0.018 ; 0.048) P-Value for Test of Close Fit (RMSEA < 0.05) = 0.97 Expected Cross-Validation Index (ECVI) = 1.97 90 Percent Confidence Interval for ECVI = (1.80 ; 2.18) ECVI for Saturated Model = 2.68 ECVI for Independence Model = 13.29 Chi-Square for Independence Model with 253 Degrees of Freedom = 2692.25 Independence AIC = 2738.25 Model AIC = 406.03 Saturated AIC = 552.00 Independence CAIC = 2837.90 Model CAIC = 761.31 Saturated CAIC = 1747.83 Normed Fit Index (NFI) = 0.90 Non-Normed Fit Index (NNFI) = 0.96 Parsimony Norrned Fit Index (PNFI) = 0.69 Comparative Fit Index (CPI) = 0.97 Incremental Fit Index (IFI) = 0.97 Relative Fit Index (RFI) = 0.87 Critical N (CN) = 193.09 Root Mean Square Residual (RMR) = 0.64 Standardized RMR = 0.090 Goodness of Fit Index (GFI) = 0.91 Adjusted Goodness of Fit Index (AGFI) = 0.87 Parsimony Goodness of Fit Index (PGFI) = 0.64 183 APPENDIX CC Q-plot of Standardized Residuals 3.5 .......................................................................... . x I x . x * . xx x * . xx iii: . x xx* . **x . xxx* . *X** . . x*xx . . **x . *x*x . *tti . xx* . tint. . x**x*. . xx* . xx*x . *x* . xxx . x** . x*x*x . xx*x . x*x . xx . x* . * . * .x x x -3.5 .......................................................................... -35 3.5 Standardized Residuals 184 REFERENCES 185 REFERENCES Ainsworth, MD. (1967). Infang in Uganda: Infant care and the gigowth of love. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press. Ainsworth, M.D.S., Blehar, M., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of attachment. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Ammaniti, M. (1991). Maternal representations during pregnancy and early infant-mother interactions. Infant Mgilal Health Journal. 12. 246-255. Ammaniti, M., Baumgartner, E., Candelori, C., Perucchini, P., Pola, M., Tambelli, R., & Zampino, F. (1992). Representations and narratives during pregnancy. _ijjapt Mental Health Joxm_1a_1,Li, 167-182. Andersen, P.A., & Telleen, S.L. (1992). The relationship between social support and maternal behaviors and attitudes: A meta-analytic review. American Journal of Communig Pachology, 20, 753-774. Bandura, A. (1977). Self-eficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. Psychological Review. 84, 191-215. Bandura, A. (1989). Regulation of cognitive processes through perceived self- eflicacy. Developmental Psychology, 25, 729-735. Barnett, D., Ganiban, J ., & Cicchetti, D. (1999). Maltreatment, negative expressivity, and the development of Type D attachments from 12 to 24 months of age. In J. Vondra & D. Barnett (Eds), Monoggaphs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 64, (3, Serial No. 258). Barnett, D., & Vondra, J .I. (1999). Atypical patterns of early attachment: Theory, research, and current directions. In J. Vondra & D. Barnett (Eds), Mononghs of the Society for Research in Child Develppment, 64, (3, Serial No. 258). Beck, A.T., Ward, C.H., Mendelson, M., Mock, J., & Erbaugh, J. (1961). An inventory for measuring depression. Archives of General Paychiagy, 4, 561-571. Belsky, J. (1999). Interactioml and contextual determinants of security. In J. Cassidy & P.R. Shaver (Eds), Handbook of Attachment (pp. 249-264). New York: The Guilford Press. Belsky, J., & Isabella, R. (1988). Maternal, infant, and social contextual determinants of attachment security. In J. Belsky & T. Nezworski (Eds), Clgp' 'cal Implications pf Attachr_r§nt (pp. 41-94). New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. 186 Benoit, D., & Parker, K. C. H. (1994). Stability and transmission of attachment across three generations. Child Development, 65, 1444-1456. Benoit, D., Parker, KC.H., & Zeanah, CH. (1997). Mothers’ representations of their infants assessed prenatally: Stability and association with infants’ attachment classifications. Journal of Child Psychology and Pachiapry, 38, 307-313. Benoit, D., Zeanah, C.H., Parker, K.C.H., Nicholson, E., & Coolbear, J. (1997). Working model of the child interview: Infant clinical status related to maternal perceptions. Infant Mental Health Jopg_1a_.l, l_8_, 107-121. Bibring, G.L., Dwyer, T.F., Huntington, D.S., & Valenstein, AF. (1961). A study of the psychological processes in pregnancy and of the earliest mother-child relationship. Psychoan_alytic Studyof the Child, 9-24. Bogat, G.A., & Levendosky, A.A. (1999). Physical and Emotional NegLept Sca_l§. Unpublished manuscript, Michigan State University. Bowlby, J. (1944). Forty-four juvenile thieves: Their characters and home life. Intepnational Journal of Psychoanalysis, 25, 107-127. Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss: Volume 1. Attachment. New York: Basic Books. Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and loss: Volume 2. Separation. New York: Basic Books. Bowlby, J. (1979). The mm and breakmg' of afiectional bonds, London: Tavistock. Bowlby, J. (1980). Attachment and loss: Volume 3. Loss. sadness. and dgpression. New York: Basic Books. Bowlby, J. (1988). L secure base: Parent child attachment and healthy development. New York: Basic Books. Bretherton, I. (1990). Communication patterns, internal working models, and the intergenerational transmission of attachment relationships. Infant Mental Health low, l_1, 237-252. Bretherton, I., Biringen, Z., Ridgeway, D., Masline, C., & Sherman, M. (1989). Attachment: The parental perspective. Infant Mental Health Joml, 10, 203-221. Bretherton, I., & Munholland, KA. (1999) Internal working models in attachment relationships: A construct revisited. In J. Cassidy & P. Shaver (Eds), Handbook of Attachment (pp. 89-111). New York: The Guilford Press. 187 Bretherton, l., Ridgeway, D., & Cassidy, J. (1990). Assessing internal working models of the attachment relationship: An attachment story completion tasks for 3 year olds. In M. Greenberg, D. Cicchetti, & E.M. Cummings (Eds), Attachment in the _meschool yea_r_'s (pp. 273-308). Chicago: The Chicago University Press. Bromwich, R. (1978). Workaig' with parents and infants. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed. Bumberry, W., Oliver, J.M., & McClure, IN. (1978). Validation of the beck depression inventory in a university population using psychiatric estimate as the criterion. Journal of Consultm’ and Clinical Psychology, 46, 150-155. Bus, A.G., & Van Ijzendoom, M.H. (1992). Patterns of attachment in fiequently and infiequently reading mother-child dyads. The Jom of Genetic Psychology, 153, 395-403. Carlson, E.A., & Sroufe, LA. (1995). Contribution of attachment theory to developmental psychopathology. In D. Cicchetti & D.J. Cohen (Eds), Developmental psychomthology, Volume 1: Theory and methods (pp. 581-617). New York: John Wiley & Sons, Inc. Cascardi, M., & O’Leary, K.D. (1992). Depressive symptomatology, self-esteem, and self-blame in battered women. Journal of Family Violence, 7, 249-259. Cassidy, J. (1988). Child-mother attachment and the self in six year olds. Child Development, 59, 121-134. Cassidy, J. (1990). Theoretical and methodological considerations in the study of attachment and the self in young children. In M. Greenberg, D. Cicchetti, & E.M. Cummings (Eds), Attachment in the preschool years (pp. 87-119). Chicago: The Chicago University Press. Cassidy, J., & Berlin, L]. (1994). The insecure/ambivalent pattern of attachment: Theory and research. Chilfi Development 65 971-991. Cassidy, J ., & Kobak, RR (1988). Avoidance and its relation to other defensive processes. In J. Belsky & T. Nezworski (Eds), Clinical Implications of Attachment (pp. 300-323). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Cicchetti, D. (2001, April). Pachopathology in lopggm' dm mtive: The MLS and beyond. Paper presented at the Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Minneapolis Minnesota. Cohen, J. (1968). Weighted kappa. Psycholpgrpd' Bulletpp' 70, 213-220. 188 Conrad, 8., Gross, D., Fogg, L., & Ruchala, P. (1992). Maternal confidence, knowledge, and quality of mother-toddler interactions: A preliminary study. Infant Mental Health Jogml, 13, 353-362. Crittenden, RM. (1985). Social networks, quality of child rearing, and child development. Child Development, 56, 1299-1313. Crittenden, RM. (1988). Relationships at risk. In J. Belsky & T. Nezworski (Eds), Clinical Implications of Attachment (pp. 136-174). New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Crnic, K.A., Greenberg, M.T., & Slough, N.M. (1986). Early stress and social support influences on mothers’ and high risk infants’ functioning in late infancy. Infant Mental Health JomaL 7, 19-32. Crockenberg, S. (1981). Infant irritability, mother responsiveness, and social support influences on the security of infant-mother attachment. Child Develme 52, 857-865. Crockenberg, S. (1988). Social support and parenting. Journal pf Behavioral Pediatrics 141-174. Crowell, J .A., Fraley, R.C., & Shaver, RR. (1999). Measurement of individual differences in adolescent and adult attachment. In J. Cassidy & P.R. Shaver (Eds), Handbook of Attachment (pp. 434-465). New York: The Guilford Press. Crowell, J.A., & Treboux, D. (1995). A review of adult attachment measures: Implications for theory and research. Social Development, 4, 294-327. Cummings, EM. (1990). Classification of attachment on a continuum of felt security. In M. Greenberg, D. Cicchetti, & E.M. Cummings (Eds), Aaachment in the praschool years (pp. 311-338). Chicago: The Chicago University Press. Cutrona, C.E., & Troutman, BR (1986). Social support, infant temperament, and parenting self-emcacy: A mediational model of postpartum depression. Child Development, 57, 1507- 1 5 1 8. DeWolfi‘, M., & van Ijzendoom, M. (1997). Sensitivity and attachment: A meta- analysis on parental antecedents of infant attachment. Chilg fivalopmgat, 68, 571-591 . Donovan, W.L., & Leavitt. LA. (1989). Maternal self-efieacy and infant attachment: Integrating physiology, perceptions, and behavior. Child Devglppmpt, 60, 460-472. 189 Donovan, W.L., Leavitt. L.A., & Walsh, R0. (1990). Maternal self-efficacy: Illusory control and its effect on susceptibility to learned helplessness. Child Development, 61, 163 8-1647. Egeland, B., & Farber, EA. (1984). Infant-mother attachment: Factors related to its development and changes over time. Child Development, 55, 753-771. Egeland, B., & Sroufe, LA. (1981). Attachment and early maltreatment. Child Developmeat. 52, 44-52. Eiden, RD., Teti, D.M., & Corns, KM. (1995). Maternal working models of attachment, marital adjustment, and the parent-child relationship. Child Development, 66, 1504-1518. Epstein, S. (1973). The self-concept revisited: Or a theory of a theory. American P§ychologist, 404-416. Epstein, S. (1983). The Mother-Father-Peer Scale. Unpublished manuscript. University of Massachusetts, Amherst. Erickson, M.F., Egeland, B., & Pianta, R. (1989). The effects of maltreatment on the development of young children. In D. Cicchetti & V. Carlson (Eds), Child maltreatment: Theog and research on the causes and consequences of child abuse and neglect (pp. 647-684). Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press. Fava Vizziello, G., Antonioli, M.E., Cocci, V., & Invernizzi, R (1993). From pregnancy to motherhood: The structure of representative and narrative change. Infant Ment_al Health Journal,_1_4, 4-16. Fleiss, J.L., Cohen, J., & Everitt, BS. (1969). Large sample standard errors of kappa and weighted kappa. chhological Bulletpa' 72, 323-327. Fonagy, P., Steele, M., Moran, G., Steele, H., & Higgitt, A. (1993). Measuring the ghost in the nursery: An empirical study of the relation between parents’ mental representations of childhood experiences and their infants’ security of attachment. Journal of th_e American Pachommc Assopiatiop, 41, 957-989. Fonagy, P., Steele, H., & Steele, M. (1991). Maternal representations of attachment during pregnancy predict the organization of infant-mother attachment at one year of age. Child Development, 62, 891-905. Fonagy, P., Steele, M., Steele, H., Moran, G.S., & Higgitt, AC. (1991). The capacity for understanding mental states: The reflective self in parent and child and its significance for security of attachment. Infant Mental Hfl mm, 12, 201-218. 190 Fox, RA. (1994). Parent Behavior Checklist. Brandon, VT: Clinical Psychology Publishing Co., Inc. Fraiberg, S., Adelson, E., & Shapiro, V. (1975). Ghosts in the nursery: A psychoanalytic approach to the problem of impaired infant-mother relationships. Journal of the American Academy of Child Paychiatg, 14, 387-422. Freud, S. (1940). An outline of psychoanalysis. SE, 23. Frodi, A., Grolnic, W., & Bridges, L. (1985). Maternal correlates of stability and change in infant-mother attachment. Infant Mental Health Jou_raal_, 6, 60-67. Gelfand, D., & Teti, D. (1990). The effects of maternal depression on children Clinical chhology Review, 10, 329-353. George, C., Kaplan, N., & Main, M. (1985). The Adult Attachment Interview. Unpublished manuscript, University of California at Berkeley. George, C., & Solomon, J. (1989). Internal working models of caregiving and security of attachment at age six. Infant Mental Health Jou_r_nal, 10, 222-237. George, C., & Solomon, J. (1996). Representational models of relationships: Links between caregiving and attachment. Infant Mental Haalth low}, 17, 198-216. Grossmann, K., Fremmer-Bombik, E., Rudolph, J., & Grossmann, KE. (1988). Maternal attachment representations as related to patterns of infant-mother attachment and maternal care during the first year. In R.A. Hinde & J. Stevenson-Hinde (Eds), Relationships \Vrthin Families (pp. 241-260). Oxford: Clarendon Press. Gunner, M.R., Mangelsdorf, S., Larson, M., & Hertsgaard, L. (1989). Attachment, temperament, and adrenocortical activity in infancy: A study of psychoendocrine regulation. Developmental chhology, 25, 355-363. Halpern, R (1993). Poverty and infant development. In C. Zeanah (Eds), Handbook of Infant Mental Hflh (pp. 73-86). New York: The Guilford Press. Hamilton, CE. (2000). Continuity and discontinuity of attachment from infancy through adolescence. Cmfl Development, 71, 690-694. Harman, K. & Luster, T. (1991). Influence of parent, child, and contextual factors on the quality of the home environment. Infant Mental Health Jom], 12, 17-30. Hesse, E. (1999). The Adult Attachment Interview: Historical and current perspectives. In J. Cassidy & P.R. Shaver (Eds), Handmok of Attachment (pp. 395-433). New York: The Guilford Press. 191 Holden, G. W., & Ritchie, KL. (1991). Linking extreme marital discord, child rearing, and child behavior problems: Evidence fi'om battered women. Child Development, 62, 311-327. Hollingshead, AB. (1975). Four factor index of social status. Unpublished manuscript. Yale University, New Haven, CT. Hoyle, RH, & Panter, AT. (1995). Writing about structural equation models. In R.H. Hoyle (Ed), Structural Equation Modehpg‘ (pp. 158-176). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications. Isabella, RA. (1994). Origins of maternal role satisfaction and its influences upon maternal interactive behavior and infant-mother attachment. Infant Bahavipr m Development, 17, 381-387. Joreskog, K., & SOrbom, D. (2001). Lm’ 18.5. Scientific Software International, Inc. Jouriles, E.N., & Norwood, W.D. (1995). Physical aggression toward boys and girls in families characterized by the battering of women. Jom of Fa-Lrly' Psychology, 2, 69-78. Leifer, M. (1977). Psychological changes accompanying pregnancy and motherhood. Genetic P chol Mono 95 55-96. Levendosky, A.A., & Graham-Bermann, SA (2001). Parenting in battered women: The effects of domestic violence on women and their children. Journal of me' Violence, 16, 171-192. Levine, L.V., Tuber, S.B., Slade, A., & Ward, M.J. (1991). Mothers’ mental representations and their relationship to mother-infant attachment. Bulletin of m Mem' er Clinic, 55, 454-469. Levitt, M.J., Weber, R.A., & Clark, M.C. (1986). Social network relationships as sources of maternal support and well-being. Davelopmenpal chhokzgy, 3, 310-316. Lichtenstein, J., & Cassidy, J. (1991, April). The inventogy of Adlat Att_ac,hmt (INVAA): Validation of a new m1: of Quit afiapflnt. Paper presented at the biennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Seattle, WA. Lieberman, A, & Pawl, J.H. (1993). Infant-parent psychotherapy. In Zeanah, C. (Ed), Handmok pf Ipfaat MM Hflth (pp. 427-442). New York: The Guilford Press. Lumley, J .M. (1982). Attitudes to the fetus among primigravidae. Amljap P ' 'c Jo 18 106-109. 192 Luster, T., & McAdoo, HP. (1994). Factors related to the achievement and adjustment of young African American children. Cad d Development, 65, 1080-1094. Lyons-Ruth, K., Bronfinan, E., & Parsons, E. (1999). Maternal fiightened, frighteining, or atypical behavior and disorganized infant attachment patterns. In J. Vondra & D. Barnett (Eds), Monogzaphs of th_e Society for Research in Child Development, 64, (3, Serial No. 258). Lyons-Ruth, K., Connell, D.B., Grunebaurn, H. Botein, S., & Zoll, D. (1984). Maternal family history, maternal caretaking and infant attachment in multiproblem families. Preveative Paychiatgy, 2, 403-425. Lyons-Ruth, K., Connell, DB, 2011, D., & Stahl, J. (1987). Infants at social risk: Relations among infant maltreatment, rmternal behavior, and infant attachment behavior. Developmental Psycholog, 23, 223-232. Lyons-Ruth, K., & Jacobvitz, D. (1999). Attachment disorganization: Unresolved loss, relational violence, and lapses in behavioral and attentional strategies. In J. Cassidy & P.R. Shaver (Eds), Handbook of Attachment (pp. 520-554). New York: The Guilford Press. Main, M., & Goldwyn, R. (1984a). Adult attachment scor_r_r_1g' and classification. Unpublished manuscript. University of California at Berkeley. Main, M., & Goldwyn, R. (1984b). Predicting rejection of her infimt from mother’s representation of her own experience: Implications for the abused-abusing intergenerational cycle. Child Abuse & Neglect, 8, 203-217. Main, M., & Goldwyn, R. (1998). Adult attachment scon_ng' and classification system. Unpublished manuscript. University of California at Berkeley. Main, M., & Hesse, E. (1990). Parents unresolved traumatic experiences are related to infant disorganized attachment status: Is frightened and/or fiighteing parental behavior the linking mechanism? In M. Greenberg, D. Cicchetti, & E.M. Cummings (Eds), Machme_nt in th_e Preschool Years (pp. 161-182). Chicago, IL: The University of Chicago Press. Main, M., Kaplan, N., & Cassidy, J. (1985). Security in infancy, childhood, and adulthood: A move to the level of representation. In Bretherton & Waters (Eds), Mpnographs of the Socieg for Research in Child Develppgnt, 50 (Serial 1-2 1. Main, M., & Solomon, J. (1990). Procedures for identifying infants as disorganized/disoriented during the Ainsworth Strange Situation. In M. Greenberg, D. Cicchetti, & E.M. Cummings (Eds), Attachment in t3 Preschool Ym (pp. 121-160). Chicago, IL: The University of Chicago Press. 193 Marshall, LL. (1992). Development of the severity of violence against women scales. Journal of Family Violence, 7, 103-121. Martins, C., & Gafl‘an, EA. (2000). Effects of early maternal depression on patterns of infant-mother attachment: A meta-analytic investigation. Journal of Child Pacholpgy and Pachiatpy, 41, 737-746. Marvin, R.S., & Britner, RA. (1999) Normative development: The ontogeny of attachment. In J. Cassidy & P. Shaver (Eds), _H_aadbook of Attachment (pp. 44-67). New York: The Guilford Press. McDonough, S. (1993). Interaction guidance: Understanding and treating early infant-caregiver relationship disturbances. In Zeanah, C. (Ed), Handmk of Infant Mental Health (pp. 414-426). New York: The Guilford Press. Mebert, C.J. (1989). Stability and change in parents’ perceptions of infant temperament: Early pregnancy to 13.5 months postpartum. Infant Behavior and Development, 12, 237-244. Mebert, C.J., & Kalinowski, M.F. (1986). Parents’ expectations and perceptions of infant temperament: “Pregnancy status” differences. Infant Behavior and Development, 9, 321-334. Mercer, RT, & Ferketick, S.L. (1990). Predictors of parental attachment during early parenthood. Journal of Advmd Nursing, 15, 268-280. Mercer, R.T., & Ferketick, S.L. (1994). Matemal-infant attachment of experienced and inexperienced mothers during pregnancy. Nursmg’ Research, 43, 344- 351. Meyer, H.J. (1988). Marital and mother-child relationships: Developmental history, parent personality, and child diflicultness. In R.A. Hinde & J. Stevenson-Hinde (Eds), Relationships within Families (pp. 119-139). Oxford: The Oxford University Press. Muller, ME. (1992). A critical review of prenatal attachment research. Scholargly Ingarg' for Nursgg’ Practice: An Intefltional Jou_rpa_l, 6, 5-22. Norbeck, J. (1995). Seomg’ instructions for the Norbeck Spcial Sppmg Qua-31W Unpublished manuscript- Norbeck, J., Lindsey, A.M., & Carrieri, V.L. (1981). The development of an instrument to measure social support. Nursn_rg' Researca 39, 264-269. 194 Norbeck, J., Lindsey, A.M., & Carrieri, V.L. (1982). Further development of the Norbeck Social Support Questionnaire: Normative data and validity testing. Nursmg' Research, 30, 4-9. Norton, D. (1990). Understanding the early experience of black children in high- risk environments: Culturally and ecologically relevant research in regard to support for families. Zero to Three 10 1-7. O’Keefe, M. (1995). Predictors of child abuse in maritally violent families. Journal of Intmnal Violence, 10, 3-25. Oppenheirn, D., & Waters, HS. (1995). Narrative processes and attachment representations: Issues of development and assessment. In Waters, Vaughn, Posada, and Kondo-Ikemura (Eds), Mpnoggphs of the Spcieg for R_e__saarch in Child Development 60, (Serial 2-3 ). Pajulo, M., Savonlahti, E., Sourander, A., Piha, J., & Helenius, H. (2001). Prenatal maternal representations: Mothers at psychosocial risk. Infant Mental Hgalta Jomal, 22, 529-544. Pederson, D.R., Gleason, K.E., Moran, G., & Bento, S. (1998). Maternal attachment representations, maternal sensitivity, and the infant-mother attachment relationship. Developmental Pwhology, 34, 925-933. Posada, G., Waters, E., Crowell, J.A., & Lay, KL. (1995). Is it easier to use a secure mother as a secure base? Attachment Q-Sort correlates of the adult attachment interview. In Waters, Vaughn, Posada, & Kondo-Ikemura (Eds), Mpnoggphs of the Socieg for Research in Child Development, 60, (SEE 2-3 ). Radke-Yarrow, M., Cummings, E.M., Kuczynski, L., & Chapman, M. (1985). Patterns of attachment in two- and three-year-olds in normal families and families with parental depression. Cu (1 Development, 56, 884-893. Richters, J.E., Waters, E., & Vaughn, BE. (1988). Empirical classification of infant-mother relationships from interactive behavior and crying during reunion. Child Develppment, 59, 512-522. Ricks, M.H. (1985). The social transmission of parental behavior: Attachment across generations. In I. Bretherton & E. Waters (Eds), Monpgtaphs of me Soctgty' for Research in Child Developragpt, 50 (Sm 1-21. Rubin, R. (1984). Maternal identity and the germ] em’ . New York: Springer. 195 Samerofl‘, A.J., Seifer, R., Barocas, R., Zax, M., & Greenspan, S. (1987). Intelligence quotient scores of 4 year old children: Social-environmental risk factors. Pediatrics 79 343-350. Schumacker, RE, & Lomax, RG. (1996). A beginner’s guide to structural equation modeh_ng' . New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Shea, E., & Tronick, El. (1988). The maternal self-report inventory: A research and clinical instrument for assessing maternal self-esteem. In H.E. Fitzgerald (Ed), Theog and Research in Bemvioral Pediatrics Volume 4 (pp. 101-139). New York, NY: Plenum Press. Slade, A., & Cohen, L.J. (1996). The process of parenting and the remembrance of things past. Infant Mental Health Jomj, 12, 217-238. Slade, A., Derrner, M., Gibson, L., Graf, F., Grunebaum, L., Reeves, M., & Sitrin, A. (1994). The Premcy Interview Codmg’ System. Unpublished manuscript. Solomon, J., & George, C. (1999). The measurement of attachment security in infancy and childhood In J. Cassidy & P.R. Shaver (Eds), Handhpok of Attacflnt (pp. 287-316). New York: The Guilford Press. Spieker, SJ. (1986). Patterns of very insecure attachment found in samples of high-risk infants and toddlers. Tppics in Earbt Childhood Smgial Educatiop, 6, 37-53. Spieker, S.J., & Booth, CL. (1988). Maternal antecedents of attachment quality. In J. Belsky & T. Nezworski (Eds), Clinical l_rnplicatiqns of Attghment (pp. 95-135). New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Steele, H., Steele, M., & Fonagy, P. (1996). Associations among attachment classifications of mothers, fathers, and their infants. CM’ d Development, 97, 541-555. Stern, D. N. (1985). The intmpnal world of the infant. New York: Basic Books. Stern, D. N. (1995). The motherhood constehtion: A unified view of tarant- W. New York: Basic Books. Straus, M. (1979). Measuring intrafamily conflict and violence: The conflict tactics scales. qumal of mm c m d; Fgm , 41, 75-88. Teti, D.M., & Gelfand, D.M. (1991). Behavioral competence among mothers of infants in the first year: The mediational role of maternal self-eficacy. Child Develppmeat, 62, 918-929. 196 Teti, D.M., Gelfand, D.M., Messinger, D., & Isabella, R. (1995). Maternal depression and the qaulity of early attachment. Developmental thplogy, 31, 364-3 76. Teti, D.M., O’Connell, M.A., & Reiner, CD. (1996). Parenting sensitivity, parental depression and child health: The mediational role of parental self-eficacy. Barbi Qavelopraeat and Parentgg' , 5, 237-250. Thompson, RA. (2000). The legacy of early attachments. Child Development, fl, 145-152. Ullman, J.B. (1996). Structural Equation Modeling. In B.G. Tabachnick and LS. Fide11(Eds), Usm‘ g Multivariate Statistics ( th_nd' Edition) (pp. 709-811). New York, NY: Harper Collins. U.S. Census Bureau. (2001). Pov eshol 2000. [On-line]. Available: www.censusgov/hhes/poverty/threshld/threshOOhtml. Van IJzendoorn, M.H. (1995). Adult attachment representations, parental responsiveness, and infant attachment: A meta-analysis on the predictive validity of the adult attachment interview. Pacfilogical Bullata' 117, 387-403. Van Ijzendoom, M.H., & Kroonenberg. (1988). Cross cultural patterns of attachment: A meta-analysis of the Strange Situation. Child Develppmeat, 59, 147-156. Verschueren, K., Marcoen, A., & Schoefs, V. (1996). The internal working model of the self, attachment, and competence in five-year olds. Child Devalomnt, Q7, 2493- 2511. Vondra, J.I., Hommerdin, K, & Shaw, D.S. (1999). Stability and change in infant attachment in a low-income sample. In J.I. Vondra & Barnett (Eds), W Smiety for Research in Child Qavalopment (3, Serial No. 258). Vondra, J.I., Shaw, D.S., Swearingen, L., Cohen, M., & Owens, EB. (2001). Attachment stability and emotional and behavioral regulation from infancy to preschool age. fiveloppgpt m Pachopathplogy, 13, 13-33. Walker, L.O., Crain, H., & Thompson, E. (1986). Mothering behavior and maternal role attainment during the postpartum period. Nursmg' Reg-amp, 35, 352-355. Ward, M.J., & Carlson, EA. (1995). Associations among adult attachment representations, maternal sensitivity, and infant-mother attachment in a sample of adolescent mothers. Child Developgat, Q, 69-79. Waters, E., Merrick, S., Treboux, D., Crowell, J., & Albersheirn, L. (2000). Attachment security in infancy and early adulthood: A twenty-year longitudinal study. W 684-689. 197 Weinfield, N.A., Sroufe, A., & Egeland, B. (2000). Attachment from infancy to early adulthood in a high-risk sample: Continuity and discontinuity and their correlates. Child Development, 71, 695-702. Weinfield, N.S., Sroufe, A., Egeland, B., & Carlson, EA. (1999) The nature of individual differences in infant-caregiver attachment. In J. Cassidy & P. Shaver (Eds), Handbook of Attachment (pp. 68-88). New York: The Guilford Press. Williams, RM., Joy, L.A., Travis, L., Gotowiec, A., Blurn-Steele, M., Aiken, L.S., Painter, S.L., & Davidson, SM. (1987). Transition to motherhood: A longitudinal study. Infant Mental Hm Jamal, 8, 251-265. Zahr, L.K. (1991). The relationship between maternal confidence and mother- infant behaviors in premature infants. Res_eatch in Nursgg' and Hfltll, 14, 279-286. Zeanah, C.H., & Anders, T.F. (1987). Subjectivity in parent-infant relationships: A discussion of internal working models. Infant Mental Health 10ml, 8, 237-250. Zeanah, C.H., & Barton, ML. (1989). Introduction: Internal representations and parent-infant relationships. Infant Mental Health Jam 10, 135-141. Zeanah, C.H., Benoit, D., Barton, M., Regan, C., Hirshberg, L.M., & Lipsett, LP. (1993). Representations of attachment in mothers and their one-year old infants. Jo_urnal of the American Acade_my of Child and Adolescent Psychiatg, 32, 278-286. Zeanah, C.H., Benoit, D., Hirshberg, L., Barton, M.L., Regan, C. (1994). Mothers’ representations of their infants are concordant with infant attachment classifications. Devglppmental Issues in Psycm and Psychology,l, 9-18. Zeanah, C.H., & Carr, S. (1990). Fetal movements and the imagined baby of pregnancy: Are they related? Journal of Raprodugtive and Infant Paychology, 8, 23-36. Zeanah, C.H., Danis, B., Hirshberg, L., Benoit, D., Miller, D., Heller, SS. (1999). Disorganized attachment associated with partner violence: A research note. mm Health Jomal 20, 77-86. Zeanah, C.H., Keener, M.A., & Anders, T.F. (1986a). Developing perceptions of temperament and their relation to mother and infant behavior. loam pf Child Psychology Qd Pychiatg, 2 2, 499-512. Zeanah, C.H., Keener, M.A., & Anders, T.F. (1986b). Adolescent mothers’ prenatal fantasies and working models of their infimts. M 193-203. Zeanah, C.H., Keener, M.A., Anders, T.F., & Vieira-Baker, CC. (1987). Adolescent mothers’ perceptions of their infants before and after birth. W of Mpsychiag, 57, 351-360. 198 Zeanah, C.H., Keener, M.A., Steward, L, & Anders, T.F. (1985). Prenatal perception of infant personality: A preliminary investigation. Journal pf the American Acadm of Cm d Psychiafl, 24, 204-210. Zeanah, C.H., Zeanah, P.D., & Stewart, L.K. (1990). Parents’ constructions of their infants’ personalities before and after birth: A descriptive study. C_h°ddl’_sy_ca_iatty and Human Development, 20, 191-206. 199 M IIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIII ll{WI/l121111111111-Jill-11111111111III/fl 3 930 72 9738