COMMUNICATING NATURAL HAZARD RISK: WARNINGS, DECISIONS AND PRECAUTIONARY BEHAVIOR By Robert E. Drost A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University In partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Geological Sciences – Doctor of Philosophy 2014 ABSTRACT COMMUNICATING NATURAL HAZARD RISK: WARNINGS, DECISIONS AND PRECAUTIONARY BEHAVIOR By Robert E. Drost This dissertation consists of four studies that investigate factors influencing individual decision-making and preferences for delivery of information during severe weather. The first study population consisted of 49 undergraduate students and focused on the role semantic and episodic memory plays during severe weather warnings (tornado), and examined how participants would react during a simulated tornado warning. The study discovered that increased knowledge of severe weather generally lead to more prudent decisions during warnings, while greater personal experience with tornadoes lead to a decrease in taking precautionary measures. The second study was made up of 181 members attending one of two Geological Society of America annual conferences. The study investigated geoscientist’s perception of climate issues and the public’s trust of science communication. The study found that scientists generally agree that anthropogenic climate change exists, however, the public’s knowledge of climate issues varies in depth and their trust of the scientific community is wavering. The limited trust of the science community is of concern since it has the potential to negatively impact the public during exposure to a variety of natural hazards. The third study explores the impact of weathercaster gesturing on viewer retention and attention to material presented during a live weather forecast. The study was composed of thirty-six undergraduate students and was viewed on Tobii T60 eye tracking equipment in order to capture gaze data of the participants. Results of the study indicate that gesturing may be acceptable during broadcasts where non-salient information is being presented since it may redirect the attention of the viewer. However, when salient information is being presented, gesturing may redirect viewer attention away from items of importance and is therefore not recommended. The fourth study examines the impact of a traditional severe weather warning on viewer attention and retention of the weather information being presented. Multiple screen elements make up a typical weather warning and may include radar imagery, live footage, weathercaster information and warning scrolls at the bottom of the television screen. A traditional televised weather warning was compared to a novel animated version and an audio only version containing nearly the same information. All versions were viewed/heard as part of an eye tracking experiment utilizing a Tobii T60 eye tracker. Eye tracking data were used to ascertain participant gaze information. Participant knowledge and experience data were collected prior to viewing the warnings. After viewing one of the three warning treatments, participant retention and preference data were collected. The study revealed that multiple onscreen elements may lead to a diverse viewer attention pattern when viewing warnings. Additionally, participants viewing the animated warning retained significantly more warning information than the other participants. The studies completed in this dissertation indicate that numerous inputs affect viewer behavior during weather warnings. It has shown that in addition to knowledge and experience, trust in the information being delivered, delivery methods, and the make-up of the warning broadcast all influence how warning information is attended to and retained by the viewer. COPYRIGHT BY ROBERT E. DROST 2014 For my parents, Arthur and Jean Drost, for pushing me to think beyond my comfort level, and my mom, Marlene Ventura, for giving me a chance for a better life. Most importantly, for my wife, Vicki Drost, for supporting my childhood dreams.   v   ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS During my course of study that has lead to this dissertation I have been the benefactor of an enormous amount of emotional and intellectual support and encouragement from my family, friends and faculty. First, I would like to show my appreciation to my advisor, Julie C. Libarkin, for having the courage to accept and support a non-traditional student and for the encouragement to realize my life goal of being a scientist. Although difficult and often times frustrating, I have grown to embrace the nuances of becoming an interdisciplinary researcher and appreciate the knowledge and experience brought by others while I completed my research. I would also like to thank my committee members who possess a wide range of experience and broadness of discipline that has not only challenged my work, but also improved upon my results by encouraging me to explore areas unknown to me. First, Tyrone Rooney, who challenged me to think like a geologist, exposed me to the world of geochemistry, and was always available to discuss the difficulties I encountered along the way; Aaron McCright, whose bold suggestions and pinpoint focus on social aspects of my work helped me think outside the box, and challenged conventional thought in my research area: and Stephen Thomas, who served on my committee for only a short time, but supplied the skill, insight and enthusiasm that supported my research direction. Although my committee was quite diverse in its’ disciplinary background, every member supported and encouraged my education and research to the greatest possible extent. The result for me was a much deeper understanding of my own discipline and research that encompassed a much broader focus enabling collaborative engagement inside and   vi   outside the academic realm. The inspiration and guidance I have received from my committee is greatly appreciated and a critical component to my success. I would like to express my appreciation to the members of the Geocognition Research Laboratory who have provided thoughtful input and support during the last five years. In particular I would like to thank Sheldon Turner, Nicole Ladue, Emily Ward and Christy Steffke, all who made difficult times less difficult and provided support on all levels during my course of study. I thank Sheldon Turner for taking time to support my geological inspirations and partaking in discussions about zombies, drilling to the mantle and insect takeovers designed to maintain our sanity. I thank Jay Trobec for providing not only videos of his on-screen persona, but also for proposing interesting topics worthy of study. I would also like to thank Jay for his conference support and willingness to fill my spot when I was unable. I would also like to thank Mark Casteel, Matt Meister and Stephen Thomas for their support and knowledge on chapter four of my dissertation. Without their novel approaches, animation skills and real-world input, the study would not have achieved its’ goals. As a special acknowledgment I would like to show my deep appreciation for the Michigan State University Graduate School (Dr. Julius Jackson), the Office of Geological Sciences (Dr. David Hyndman), the Environmental Science and Policy Program (Dr. Jinhua Zhao), the College of Natural Science (Dr. Richard Schwartz) and Dr. Julie Libarkin for the financial and emotional support I received during my final year when life attempted to interfere with my dreams through illness. I will always remember   vii   and appreciate the genuine outpouring of concern and selflessness I experienced during that time. The actions of these individuals support and reinforce my convictions that Michigan State University is without equal among educational institutions. Finally, I especially thank my wife Vicki, my kids Ian, Megan, Kevin, Brandi and Nick for their patience, willingness to listen to ongoing “geology and meteorology” discussions around the campfire, enthusiasm for my research and for giving up a little “normality” in their life to support my decision to return to school and become a scientist.   viii   TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES xi LIST OF FIGURES xii Introduction 1 Chapter 1 Memory and decision-making: Determining action when the sirens sound. 1.1 Introduction 1.2 Theoretical Foundations 1.3 Methods 1.3.1 Participants 1.3.2 Data Collection 1.3.3 Data Analysis 1.3.4 Validity and Reliability 1.4 Results 1.5 Discussion and Conclusions ACKNOWLEDGMENTS APPENDICES Appendix 1A: Semantic Questionnaire (SQ1) Appendix 1B: Episodic Questionnaire (EQ1) Appendix 1C: Decision-making questionnaire (D1 and D2) Appendix 1D: Statement of Reprint REFERENCES 7 Chapter 2 GSA members on climate change: Where, what, and ways forward? 2.1 Introduction 2.2 Methods 2.3 Results 2.4 Discussion and Conclusions APPENDIX REFERENCES 36 36 37 37 38 42 44 Chapter 3 Gesturing During Weathercasts: A “hands down” best practice? 3.1 Introduction 3.2 Methods 3.2.1 Data Collection 3.2.2 Data Analysis 3.3 Results 3.4 Discussion and Conclusions APPENDICES Appendix 3A: Retention Survey Questions Appendix 3B: Analytical Methods 46 47 51 51 54 56 61 66 67 68   ix   8 9 12 12 13 15 16 17 23 26 27 28 30 31 32 33 REFERENCES 69 Chapter 4 Severe Weather Warning Communication: Factors impacting audience attention and retention of information during warnings. 4.1 Introduction 4.2 Methods 4.2.1 Data Collection 4.2.2 Data Sources 4.2.3 Data Analysis 4.3 Results 4.3.1 Participant Results – Knowledge, Experience and Trust 4.3.2 Model Results – Retention and Preference 4.4 Discussion and Conclusions APPENDICES Appendix 4A: Knowledge Questionnaire (KNOWLEDGEQ) Appendix 4B: Experience Questionnaire (EXPERIENCEQ) Appendix 4C: Retention Questionnaire (RETENTIONQ) Appendix 4D: Traditional Model Questionnaire (PREFERENCEQ - TRAD) Appendix 4E: Animated Model Questionnaire (PREFERENCEQ - ANIM) Appendix 4F: Audio Model Questionnaire (PREFERENCEQ - AUDIO) Appendix 4G: Independent T-test Results Appendix 4H: Analytical Methods REFERENCES 73 74 80 82 83 85 89 89 91 97 102 103 104 105 106 108 110 111 113 115   x   LIST OF TABLES Table1.1: Factor loadings and communalities for the Ignore Warning Scale. The strength of the factor loadings and number of items per factor suggest the retention of one scale, the “Ignore Warning Scale”. 15 Table 1.2: Participants (n=49) and their personal lifetime warning and event experiences. Results indicate a significant experience with severe weather warnings and a fair amount of experience with tornadoes. 19 Table 1.3: Results of the Related Samples Wilcoxon Signed Rank Test indicating that the stimulus had significant impact on the low episodic/low semantic and low episodic/high semantic groups. 20 Table 1.4: Results of the MANOVA analysis suggesting the main effects were not significant but the interaction of the two were somewhat significant given the small number of participants. 21 Table 1.5: Pre-stimulus and post stimulus results illustrating the participants likelihood of ignoring a warning for both D1 and D2. 22 Table 2.1: Survey questions and response information. Please contact authors for access to full survey results. 37 Table 3.1: Definitions of gaze data used in this study. 50 Table 3.2: AOI letter designation and corresponding areas in pixels (units squared). *Areas are identical except for the Weathercaster Hand AOI. 56 Table 3.3: Total fixations, average fixation duration and total fixation duration for both No gesture and Gesture conditions. No Gesture n=19, Gesture n=17. 58 Table 3.4: Results of the Mann-Whitney U Test Statistics for No Gesture and Gesture conditions. 59 Table 4.1: Data source descriptions and timing during the study. 82 Table 4.2: Likert scale questions and participant’s (n=90) responses. 1 = strongly agree, 5 = strongly disagree. 90 Table 4.3: Participant’s Likert scale responses to the difficulty and ease of understanding their severe weather warning model. 1=not difficult, 5=very difficulty. 94 Table 4G: Independent T-Test Results. 112   xi   LIST OF FIGURES Figure 1.1: Experiment progression and timing of the questionnaires and slide show stimulus. 14 Figure 1.2: Participants primary source of weather information. The Internet was the primary source of weather information, followed by TV with friends and family (F/F) ranking third. 18 Figure 1.3: D1 and D2 results showing the interaction of experience and knowledge when faced with the decision of ignoring a tornado warning. 20   Figure 2.1: Image showing the regions of most climate change concerns among GSA respondents. Darker orange areas are where more respondents shaded in. 38 Figure 3.1: A - No Gesture condition forecast variation and B - Gesture condition forecast variation. Both forecasts were identical except for the presence or absence of the weathercaster’s gesturing. Forecasts provided by KELO-TV, Jay Trobec, Chief Meteorologist. 52 Figure 3.2: A - No Gesture condition with associated AOI and B - Gesture condition with associated AOI. All but one AOI are identical; the Hands AOI varies based on main gesturing position in the video. 56 Figure 3.3: A and B are example gaze plots during No Gesture condition and Gesture condition forecast variations. Each depiction illustrates one participant’s gaze during each forecast. In Figure 3 A is a gaze plot for Participant Elab2011020a during the No Gesture condition forecast variation and B shows a gaze plot for Participant Elab2011006b during the Gesture condition forecast variation. 60 Figure 3.4: Example heat maps for the No Gesture condition (A) and Gesture condition (B) forecast variations. Each depiction illustrates the study population’s gaze during each forecast. In Figure 3.4 A is a heat map for study participants during the No Gesture condition forecast variation and B is a heat map for study participants during the Gesture condition forecast variation. 61 Figure 4.1: Example of Traditional and Animated media clips. The Audio only clip contains no images and is not illustrated here. 81 Figure 4.2: Series of images showing participant gaze for both the TRADITIONAL (Trad) and ANIMATED (Anim) media clip. Individual participant’s gaze is indicated by the black dots. Clip Trad1 is representative of a “diffuse” gaze pattern; Trad2 is representative of a “concentrated” gaze pattern. Clip Anim1 is representative of a gaze “concentrated” on multiple elements; Anim2 is representative of a “concentrated” gaze pattern. 96   xii   Introduction Severe weather warnings provide the projected track of a storm and its’ intensity with the intent of providing adequate lead-time so individuals may take precautionary measures to ensure personal safety and protection of property. Ideally, severe weather warnings would be 100% accurate, consistently provide adequate time for preparation, and include instructions of precautionary measures necessary to protect life and property. In addition, the public would heed these warnings and advice without question regardless of personal intuition. Unfortunately, severe weather warnings often lead to “false alarms” due to the unpredictable nature of these events. False alarms may lead to public desensitization of the severe event and result in the public ignoring the warning or substantially downplaying its’ threat potential. Individual decisions and behavior in response to a severe weather warning is often unpredictable due to an individual’s amount of knowledge and experience with severe weather as well as how the information is being conveyed during the warning. The science and improvement of forecasting and issuance of severe weather warnings remains in the hands of institutions dedicated to this purpose. Discovering what influences public behavior and determining the best methods to convey severe weather warning information becomes the variable factor that influences what precautionary measures are taken by individuals during warnings. In order to clarify what influences individual behavior, a few questions become apparent: a) how does personal experience and knowledge of severe weather affect decisions made during severe weather events? b) what information does an individual find useful when making decisions during severe weather events? c) does the amount   1   of information conveyed to an individual impact the severe warning message? and d) what role does the weathercaster serve when delivering the severe weather warning? The National Oceanic and Atmospheric Administration and the National Weather Service are dedicated to improving the effectiveness of severe weather warnings. Constant refinement of warnings designed to increase the effectiveness take place within these agencies. Recently, storm based warnings that use polygon boundaries instead of county boundaries to more precisely define areas at risk was implemented to reduce warning area and economic impact associated with unnecessary warnings. Additionally, impact based warnings are being piloted that incorporate harsher tones and more direct warning statements in an attempt to combat public complacency. Success of both measures has been hard to define. Storm based warnings remain limited when multiple warnings within a county are issued, necessitating additional clarification be made to the public. Impact based warnings graphically communicate storm potential, however they risk frightening the public with bold statements intended to elicit public response to take protective measures. Both of these warning enhancements necessitate the need for an increase in public understanding of severe weather and associated warnings. This increase in understanding may be accomplished through additional knowledge of threatening weather and more effective communication techniques. The present course of study investigates variables associated with building better severe weather warnings and delivery methods that lead to prudent precautionary measures by individuals faced with a severe event. It is intended to answer the following question: How does an individual’s knowledge and experience of severe weather, as   2   well as warning communication methods influence individual behavior and decisionmaking during a severe weather event? Four studies have been completed in order to investigate this research question. The first chapter looks at the roles of memory (semantic and episodic) in influencing an individual’s behavior during tornado warnings. Semantic memory represents the accumulated knowledge an individual has on a particular subject area. Episodic memory represents the individual’s personal experience. The second chapter investigates the science community’s (expert) attitudes and opinions on public communication of potential natural hazards using climate change as an example, as well as public trust of the science community. Chapter three investigates the impacts of gesturing (body movement by the weathercaster) and the role of TV screen clutter (multiple on screen media communicating similar information) on participant’s attention and retention during weather broadcasts. Chapter four looks at screen clutter and identifies the most helpful features during a warning and determines viewer reliance on certain modes of warning methods (scroll, radar, meteorologist recommendations). In addition, alternate delivery formats, traditional TV broadcasts and animation are used to develop novel approaches to alternative warnings. Chapter 1: Memory of past events and specific subject knowledge of severe weather influences individual behavior during severe weather events. This study examined how participants would behave during a simulated tornado warning (a 5 minute slide show stimulus showing tornadoes and resulting damage) and compared their decisions based on the amount of weather knowledge and personal experience. The study consisted of forty-nine undergraduate students and lasted 1 hour during which they were faced with   3   decision-making tasks associated with severe weather. Two major outcomes surfaced from the results: (1) Students who possessed a greater amount of weather knowledge where more likely to take precautionary measures during a severe weather event than those with less knowledge, and (2) personal experience with severe weather combined with a less amount of knowledge tended to cause the students to be less likely to take precautionary measures during a severe weather event. Chapter 2: Public perception of inconsistencies among scientist’s views and the lack of effective communication impacts public perception of science issues. Potential lack of trust and limited knowledge impact the public’s comprehension of a multitude of geoevents, from climate change to severe weather. This study examined commonalities among scientists and the public as they view natural hazards associated with climate change. These served as an indicator to the overall level of trust the public has in science based information. The public typically receives this information through local media outlets. Surveys were collected in an annual meeting of the Geological Society of America (GSA) in 2009 and again in 2011 resulting in a total of 181 surveys completed by conference attendees. Geoscientists were asked to express their views on climate change and public perception as well as their understanding of potential issues surrounding natural hazards and public communication. Major outcomes of the study indicate that while scientists accept that anthropogenic climate changes exist, the public’s knowledge is limited and a trust issues exists between the public and the scientific community. Mainly, the public perceives a lack of agreement among the scientific community on a multitude of natural hazard topics even though they represent the main source of science-based issues. Additional methods of communication need to   4   be explored in order to ensure effective public understanding of science-based issues and to bridge the gap of public perception of disagreement among scientists. Chapter 3: Televised media is one of the most accessed sources of weather information. Despite the commonalities of information being presented by weathercasters, individual preferences for delivery of weather information varies among weather broadcasts. This study examined the impact of weathercaster gesturing during weather forecasts on public retention of the information being presented. In this study, thirty-six undergraduate students were exposed to two identical weather forecasts. Both forecasts were viewed on the Tobii T60 eye tracker in order to determine participant fixation and gaze information. In one of the forecasts the weathercaster remained relatively motionless, while in the other forecast the weathercaster gestured using his hands to direct attention to the forecast. Results indicated that while gesturing diverted viewer attention from specific on screen displays, retention of the forecast material remained unaffected. Chapter 4: Wide ranges of natural disasters occur in the United States, inflicting untold personal and economic damage. Severe weather, in various forms, is experienced almost daily throughout the United States. Given the number of traditional formats, varied amount and type of information conveyed during severe weather warnings, public attention to these potential disasters is often lacking. The final study compared traditional warnings along with an animated warning in order to assess viewer impact, preference and retention of the information being presented. The study was comprised of 90 participants consisting of college students, faculty and staff. Participants completed questionnaires that gauged their level of weather knowledge and personal   5   experience. Next, they viewed one of three possible severe weather warning video clips presented on a Tobii T60 eye tracker. After viewing the nearly identical clips, a retention questionnaire was completed followed by a questionnaire designed to discover the participant’s weather warning preferences and usefulness of on-screen information presented during the clip. Of the three warning clips (traditional, animated and voice only), participants retained significantly more information during the animated clip. In addition, gaze patterns were more diffuse during the traditional clip compared to the animated clip. Conclusions: The studies completed in this dissertation found that an individual’s knowledge and experience (memories) impact behavior during severe weather warnings. However, in addition to an individual’s memories, a multitude of factors also influence the individual’s comprehension of and attention to severe weather warnings. Decision-making and resulting behavior may be impacted by not only the communication techniques and delivery method utilized by the media, but also an individual’s preferences for information received during a warning. Memory, social, cognitive and policy inputs all share a role in the understanding and resulting behavior associated when a severe weather warning is issued. Future studies involving an evaluation of an individual’s weather knowledge and experience should consider the role of screen clutter, viewer preferences, usefulness of the information being delivered, and weathercaster delivery techniques. Together this information may be used to “build” a better severe weather warning that is more effective in delivering the information and imparting the motivation to move the public to take precautionary measures insuring their safety and property.   6   Chapter 1 Memory and decision-making: Determining action when the sirens sound. Memories, both semantic, or learned knowledge, and episodic, or personal experiences, play an important role in an individual’s decision-making under risk. In addition, varying levels of knowledge and experience exist in each individual. These memories enable individuals to make informed decisions based on previous knowledge or experience, and ultimately influence one’s behavior under risk. In this study, fortynine undergraduate students participated in a one-hour, classroom-based experiment focusing on decision-making. The sample contained n=23 “episodic” participants, referred to as “high episodic”, who reported having personally experienced a tornado and n=24 participants, referred to as “low episodic” who had no reported tornado experience. Incomplete data reported by the remaining participants was not included in this study. All participants completed a decision-making task both before and after viewing a 5-minute slide show stimulus related to tornadoes and associated damage. This decision-making task prompted participants to describe the actions they would anticipate taking during an actual tornado warning. Prior to the stimulus, high episodic participants exhibited a marginally higher tendency to ignore a tornado warning than those participants without episodic, (low episodic) memories. After the tornado stimulus, all participants reported a greater likelihood to engage in precautionary action than reported prior to the stimulus. We also find that: 1) those participants with low episodic memory showed greater precaution than the high episodic memory group; and 2) participants with greater knowledge of tornadoes showed the greatest gains in anticipated precautionary behavior. This study suggests that increasing a population’s   7   general knowledge of tornadoes could result in greater individual precaution and overall safety during a tornadic event. 1.1 Introduction The National Weather Service (NWS) is tasked with providing the public with information about marine and atmospheric conditions throughout the U.S. and its territories. One specific role of the NWS is the forecasting of severe weather and the administration of warnings in ”the United States, its territories, adjacent waters and ocean areas, for the protection of life and property…” (NOAA 2010). Generating a weather warning draws on a wide range of expertise involving science, technology, social interaction, public policy, and related domains. Although significant effort has gone into developing issuance criteria used to determine when a severe weather warning is warranted (NOAA 2010), how the public utilizes warning information in the face of a hazardous event is unclear (Lazo, et. al. 2009). Researchers have also acknowledged the importance of prior experiences and knowledge in decision-making during weather warnings, although their specific influences are not as well investigated (Dillon 2011; Leiserowitz 2006). Current research involving other types of natural hazards have also identified the relationship between knowledge/experience and decision-making and believe it to be a reasonable approach when investigating behaviors associated with natural hazard risk (Morss 2008 and Whitmarsh 2008). This study specifically addresses the influence of prior experiences, termed “episodic memories” here, and knowledge, termed “semantic memories” here, during an artificial weather warning. Episodic memories represent actual life experiences, and likely have both knowledge and affective components (Tulving 1972). Semantic   8   memories are information about the world that has been learned through reading, media, schooling, and other indirect experiences (Tulving 1972). Specifically, we designed a classroom experiment that probed both their semantic and episodic memories and gave participants a sense of immediacy, as if a real tornado warning were being issued. This experiment addressed the central research question: “What influences do varying levels of semantic and episodic memories have in influencing decision-making about tornado warnings?” We hypothesized that those individuals with personal memories of a tornado would be more inclined to take precautionary measures than those without such experience; however, results did not support that outcome. When an individual is faced with a risk, both types of memory play an active role during analytical and experiential processing of associated decisions, since decisions draw upon past memories (Marx 2007). Constraints on time, knowledge, and cognitive load are vital when determining a course of action in risk situations. These impact an individual’s potential options when responding to warnings by limiting the opportunity to integrate past experiences and information into decision-making. 1.2 Theoretical Foundations A number of studies have suggested a link between experiences, knowledge and decision-making under risk exists. In fact, people have a tendency to make decisions based on small amounts of previous experience (Hertwig & Pleskac 2010). Navigating risk situations requires a complete understanding and assessment of the risk and needs to take into consideration previous experiences and other sources of information impacting the context of the situation (Kasperson 1996). When exposed to risk, individuals call upon a wide range of tools in order to assess potential danger, and   9   determine the appropriate behavior. Often the level of an individual’s knowledge of the risk, as well as the fear associated with the actual event itself, will be considered before taking action (Morrow 2009). People are exposed to a variety of risk situations as part of their daily routine. These situations vary greatly, ranging from business decisions to medical predicaments, all of which are impacted by experiences and knowledge. Studies of business managers have shown that decisions often follow intuitive paths, or are highly context dependent. Management decisions can also be based on an individual’s fear of being wrong, lack of a common decision-making structure across groups of individuals, or insufficient knowledge (Riabacke 2006). Medical decisionmaking is another area that can require individuals to make decisions under risk. Pregnancy, for example, is a situation in which individuals are presented with information typically categorized as a high or low risk. Prior experience with pregnancy alleviates some of the unfamiliarity associated with decision-making around risks, although differences in pregnancies require an individual to evaluate risk in the context of the current medical condition (Lyerly et, al. 2007). Many of these same factors, such as knowledge, previous experience, context, and the perceived level of risk influence decisions encountered during exposure to natural hazards. Decision-making under natural hazard risk is similar to risk encountered in other situations. Uncertainty arises due to lack of knowledge, in this case of natural hazards, as well as the unpredictability associated with the systems and processes that influence natural hazards. The way in which these hazards impact society and an individual’s choices related to the hazard is often difficult to ascertain due to the information and   10   outcomes of past events that are available, many which may represent an imperfect record of past events (UKCIP 2003). Both previous experience, or episodic memory, and knowledge, or semantic memory, lie at the core of decisions made during risk situations. As a result, this study will take a closer look at how each impacts decisions made during exposure to risk associated with tornado warnings. For the purposes of this study, we will focus on specific memories, both episodic and semantic, and attempt to determine the role they play when an individual is exposed to tornado warnings. During decision-making tasks, people may draw on semantic knowledge and/or episodic experience as they make decisions based on their available memories. When attempting to determine the behavior associated with these memories, it is difficult to predict the outcome since decidedly different scenarios may occur despite similar past experience or knowledge. In this study, we hypothesized that someone who has experienced a tornado will act upon the warning by heeding the message to take precautionary measures. Conversely, those individuals who have not previously experienced a tornado and have little knowledge of the actual threat may choose to ignore the warning. These hypotheses will also take into account the impact of an individual’s knowledge of severe weather and tornadoes, since it has been shown to influence decisions (Johnson 1992), and the likelihood that an inverse relationship between the risk and benefits associated taking precautionary measures during a tornado warning may exist (Slovic 2000).   11   1.3 Methods A purposeful sample of students participated in this study, completing four questionnaires and participating in a brief stimulus. The first was the Semantic Severe Weather Questionnaire (SQ1), which determined the student’s level of knowledge about severe weather. Next was the Episodic Severe Weather Questionnaire (EQ1), which determined the student’s personal experience with severe weather, in general, and specifically tornadoes. Following the completion of SQ1 and EQ1, participants completed a four-point Likert scale Severe Weather Decision Questionnaire (D1), followed by a slide show stimulus, then completion of the final Severe Weather Decision Questionnaire (D2). Questionnaire data provided: 1) semantic knowledge of severe weather (SQ1); 2) personal experience with severe events and tornadoes (EQ1); and 3) potential responses to tornado warnings, collected both before (D1) and after (D2) viewing of a slideshow stimulus consisting of tornadoes and related damage. The intent of the stimulus was to determine the impact of a recent experience on decision-making, by either enabling prior recollection, or creating a recent experience. 1.3.1 Participants Forty-nine undergraduate students at a large Midwestern institution participated in a one-hour, cognitively based experiment focusing on decision-making during a regularly scheduled classroom time and location. The experimental population averaged 22 years in age and was composed of male (n=21) and female (n=28) participants. The population consisted of varying levels of academic completion (freshman, sophomore, junior and senior), all of whom were non-science majors.   12   1.3.2 Data Collection Data were collected in four successive steps during class (Figure 1). Participants were introduced to the cognitive definitions of semantic and episodic memory, given a brief explanation of the experiment, and informed that participation was voluntary. A two-page semantic questionnaire (SQ1) was then administered. The questionnaire consisted of five demographic, three informational and ten weather knowledge questions mainly centered on severe thunderstorms and tornadoes. The episodic questionnaire (EQ1) followed and consisted of seven questions focusing on personal experience with severe weather and tornadoes. After completing the semantic and episodic questionnaires, the students were asked to complete a decision-making questionnaire (D1). The decision-making questionnaire prompted students to consider their actions during a tornado warning, and listed ten potential scenarios of varying degrees of precautionary measures for students to choose from, and posed two openended questions. D1 was intended to determine actions participants would most likely take during a tornado warning. Upon completion of these questionnaires, the participants viewed a 5-minute slide show stimulus containing a series of images of actual tornadoes and associated property damage. Participants were asked to immerse themselves in the stimulus as if they and their families were actually experiencing the tornadoes both physically and emotionally. The slide show stimulus was intended to invoke a personal experience, or act as a priming event in the case of those with previous tornado experiences. After viewing the slide show, the students responded anew to the decision-making questionnaire (D2). The intent of D2 was to determine the impact of the stimulus on the decision-making process. The session ended with a   13   deeper explanation of the experiment and a question-and-answer period with the participants. Figure 1.1: Experiment progression and timing of the questionnaires and slide show stimulus. We investigated the factorability of the ten items contained within the decisionmaking instrument (D1) through exploratory factor analysis (Table 1.1). Criteria demonstrating factorability were met, including a Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin measure of sampling adequacy of 0.733, above the 0.6 value recommended for factor analysis. Bartlett’s Test of sphericity was significant (χ2 (45) = 257.4, p < 0.001). Communalities were at or above 0.5, except for one just below that value, indicating shared variance with other items. Given these data, exploratory factor analysis was performed on all ten items. A one factor solution was indicated by scree plot analysis, while eigenvalue analysis suggests a three factor solution. Ultimately, after considering the strength of factor loadings, number of items per factor, and internal consistency of factors, we retained one scale, the “Ignore Warning Scale”, which contains four items (5,6,7,9;   14   Table 1.1) and which explains 80.1% of the variance in these four items. This scale has high Cronbach’s alpha of 0.90. A high score on this scale indicates that a respondent is likely to ignore a tornado warning, while a low score indicates a less likely probability that they would ignore the warning. Confirmatory factor analysis on the same items after the stimulus (D2) confirms the stability of this scale, with equivalent factor loadings and alpha values at both administrations. Table 1.1: Factor loadings and communalities for the Ignore Warning Scale. The strength of the factor loadings and number of items per factor suggest the retention of one scale, the “Ignore Warning Scale”. Item Factor Loading Communalities Ignore warning since you see no .928 .861 apparent danger Ignore warning since you assume .941 .885 you are safe in your current location Ignore warning since there’s a small chance that you will be hit by a .931 .867 tornado Wait for sirens to sound before .664 .441 seeking shelter 1.3.3 Data Analysis Participant questionnaire responses were collected and analyzed quantitatively. First, we describe the results of the semantic and episodic questionnaire using simple descriptive statistics. Second, a Related Samples Wilcoxon Signed Rank Test was performed on the decision results both before and after the stimulus. The test is useful when investigating any change in scores from one time point to another when individuals are subjected to more than one condition, which in this case was the stimulus. We considered results for four different populations: the high semantic, high episodic group, the high semantic, low episodic group, the low semantic, high episodic group, and the low semantic, low episodic group. Based on these results, a multivariate   15   analysis of variance (MANOVA) was performed to determine the significance of the main and interaction effects suggested by the Related Samples Wilcoxon Signed Rank Test results. 1.3.4 Validity and Reliability Quantitative research enables a researcher to ask specific questions, generate hypotheses, collect data in a controlled setting, and subsequently test hypotheses (Golafshani 2003). Validity in quantitative research is dependent on the ability to measure and observe the intended variables and reliability is implied when the experiment and resulting data can be replicated. Criteria for validity and reliability of our survey instruments and study design have been established. We explain each of these criteria in detail; see Clark and Libarkin (2011) and references therein for more information. Note that overall, the findings of the current study are limited to collegeaged populations in the Midwest. Content validity is the extent to which questions are actually measuring the construct intended. Often, experts are asked to comment on item validity. In this case, materials generated by two expert organizations, NOAA and NWS, were used in developing all three survey instruments. The D1 and D2 utilized information from the NOAA and NWS tornado preparedness guidelines (NOAA 1995). The EQ1 incorporated items modified from the Enhanced Fujita Scale (NOAA 2009). The SQ1 included items generated by NOAA and NWS (NOAA 1995 and NWS 1995), as well as misconceptions identified in the research literature (NOAA 1995). All three surveys were revised based on comments from two geoscientists from the Geocognition Research Laboratory.   16   Conclusion validity is the ability to determine the relationship between variables being researched. Experts exposed to the findings of this research through formal presentations at professional conferences found the results noteworthy and deserving of further investigation. In general, experts agreed that our finding of a relationship between knowledge/experience and decision-making was enlightening; this concurs with current research on other types of natural hazards (Morss 2008 and Whitmarsh 2008). Construct validity refers to the ability of instruments to correlate with the underlying theory of the study. Current understanding of the importance of knowledge in decision-making suggests that higher knowledge should lead to more effective decision-making (Johnson 1992). In general, participants with higher SQ1 scores displayed more effective tornado-warning decisions relative to those with lower SQ1 scores. This effect was mitigated by the impact of personal experience, the focus of this study. Reliability of the instruments was evaluated through demonstrated consistency of tests results with similar populations utilizing comparable instruments. The small sample size limits internal reliability considerations, although ongoing work will allow for comparison of findings across populations. 1.4 Results Results of the Severe Weather Semantic Questionnaire (SQ1) indicate that participants were on average “somewhat likely” to consult a weather forecast as part of their daily routine. Participants checked weather forecasts an average of 4.9 times each week (approximately once daily), and referenced a wide range of sources for their   17   information about current weather (Figure 1.2). Results also indicate that 63% of participants understood the difference between the National Weather Service (NWS) severe weather statements. The two most prominent statements in this study, tornado watch and tornado warning, should prompt different levels of action; a watch suggesting conditions exist for tornado development, and a warning indicating a tornado exists. While the majority of participants understood that a “warning” was the most urgent NWS statement and required immediate personal action, a substantial number of participants (37%), were oblivious to the difference, and would not realize the need for taking precautionary measures during a tornado warning. Based on the results of SQ1, participants were divided into two groups determined by their SQ1 score. Those participants who averaged below the overall average, which happened to be 50%, were classified as low semantic (LS), and those who scored above 50% were classified as high semantic (HS). Figure 1.2: Participants primary source of weather information. The Internet was the primary source of weather information, followed by TV with friends and family (F/F) ranking third.   18   Results of the Episodic Severe Weather Questionnaire (EQ1) indicate that students had a significant amount of experience with severe weather and tornadoes. Responses to two questions, “Have you personally experienced a severe weather event” (EpiQ3), and “Have you personally experienced a tornado” (EpiQ5), indicated that 88% of the participants had personal experience with severe weather warnings and events, while slightly fewer than 50% had experienced a tornado (Table 1.2). These questions were used as indicators of overall severe weather and tornado episodic experience. Participants who indicated no experience with tornadoes were classified as low episodic (LE), and those participants who indicated experience with at least one tornado were classified as high episodic (HE). Since this study concerns itself specifically with tornado experience, the statistical analysis incorporated only responses to EpiQ5, which specifically asks the participant to state their tornado experience. Table 1.2: Participants (n=49) and their personal lifetime warning and event experiences. Results indicate a significant experience with severe weather warnings and a fair amount of experience with tornadoes. Severe Weather Event Tornado Event % of participants Severe Weather Warning 88% 84% 47% # of experiences 3.4 per year 31.0 lifetime 1.4 lifetime The main and interaction effects of knowledge of severe weather impacts and varying levels of personal experience with tornadoes on decision-making before and after a simulated warning (stimulus) were considered. Essentially, the interaction allowed for insight into the impact of knowledge and experience when students were faced with a decision that involved ignoring a tornado warning. The impact of the interaction emphasized the greater likelihood of participants in the high semantic group towards taking precautionary measures during tornado warnings (Figure 1.3).   19   Figure 1.3: D1 and D2 results showing the interaction of experience and knowledge when faced with the decision of ignoring a tornado warning. For the Wilcoxon Signed Rank Test, we tested the hypothesis that no difference exists between the mean of D1 and D2. The test is often used when comparing two related samples, which in our case is D1 and D2. The test results indicated that those possessing low episodic and low semantic memories and low episodic and high semantic memories both experienced significant impacts due to the stimulus. Those possessing high episodic and low semantic memories and high episodic and high semantic memories experienced only moderate change after the stimulus (Table 1.3). Table 1.3: Results of the Related Samples Wilcoxon Signed Rank Test indicating that the stimulus had significant impact on the low episodic/low semantic and low episodic/high semantic groups. Related Samples Wilcoxon Signed Rank Test Group N D1 D2 Statistical significance Low episodic 11 1.5 1.1 p< 0.016 Low semantic Low episodic 14 1.1 0.62 p < 0.007 High semantic   20   Table 1.3 (cont’d) High episodic 7 Low semantic High episodic 16 High semantic All 47 1.0 0.89 p < 0.180 1.4 1.1 p < 0.066 1.3 0.93 p < 0.001 A multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) was completed in order to assess the statistical significance of the independent variables “episodic” and “semantic” and the interaction term “episodic*semantic” on the dependent variables D1 and D2. This was chosen since the MANOVA allows us to investigate whether changes in the independent variables impact multiple dependent variables, and offer insight into the interactions between dependent and independent variables. MANOVA results suggested that the main effects, semantic and episodic memory were not significant with semantic D1=.953, D2=.638, and episodic D1=.654, D2=.521. However, the interaction of semantic and episodic, D1=.128, D2=.174, memory may be considered somewhat significant given the relatively small number of participants and the overall difference in significance indicated by the test (Table 1.4). Table 1.4: Results of the MANOVA analysis suggesting the main effects were not significant but the interaction of the two were somewhat significant given the small number of participants.   21   In all, participant responses to SQ1 and EQ1, question EpiQ5, indicate interesting interactions between knowledge and prior tornado experience and anticipated responses to tornado warnings. These responses were compared using the previously identified “choose to ignore” warning scale. Participant decision-making prestimulus (D1) indicates that the HE group with a high level of knowledge (HS) was more likely to take precautionary measures during a tornado warning than the HE group with a low level of knowledge (LS), while the LE group with a low level of knowledge (LS) was more likely to take precautionary measures than the LE group with a high level of knowledge (HS). The interaction between semantic and episodic memory was significant after the stimulus. Participant decision-making post stimulus (D2) resulted in interesting movements among the groups. The HE group with a high level of knowledge (HS) was found to be more likely to ignore a tornado warning than the HE group with a low level of knowledge (LS), while the LE group with a high level of knowledge was less likely to ignore a tornado warning than the LE group with a low level of knowledge (LS). It appears that tornado experience may provide impetus for participants to take precautionary measures, especially when combined with a high level of knowledge of severe weather/tornadoes (Table 1.5). It is not known, however, how long the impact of recent memories, or priming of existing memories, of tornado experiences provides this effect. Table 1.5: Pre-stimulus and post stimulus results illustrating the participants likelihood of ignoring a warning for both D1 and D2. D1 Pre-stimulus D2 Post stimulus Likelihood of Likelihood of Ignoring Ignoring Experience Knowledge Experience Knowledge Tornado Tornado level level level level Warning Warning 1=more likely 1=more likely   22   Table 1.5 (cont’d) High episodic High episodic Low episodic Low episodic High semantic Low semantic High semantic Low semantic High episodic High episodic Low episodic Low episodic 3 1 2 4 High semantic Low semantic High semantic Low semantic 1 3 4 2 1.5 Discussion and Conclusions Data analyzed from the study points to interesting interactions between knowledge and experience. Varying levels of severe weather knowledge coupled with instances, or lack of, personal tornado experience allow for a wide range of potential outcomes when an individual is faced with a tornado warning. Although the study was limited by availability of prior research in the topic area as well as sample size, (n=49), and the relatively narrow scope of the student demographics, the results are still considered attributable to cognitive and demographic factors that substantiate the need for further research using refined methods and more diverse participants. In general, this study suggests that participants with a higher level of semantic knowledge (HS) will be more likely to take precautionary measures during a tornado warning, than those who scored lower on the semantic questionnaire (LS). In addition, students who have previously experienced a tornado (HE) are the least likely to react favorably to a warning if they possess less knowledge of severe weather. This may be due to the possibility of the students discounting the apparent danger and relying on the greater availability of relatively harmless past experiences. After the stimulus, the episodic group increased their likeliness to react to the warning, but only slightly. The results tend to support the likelihood of semantic memories, or knowledge, providing   23   greater impetus for action during a severe weather event, when compared to only episodic memories, or experiences of tornadoes. Both semantic and episodic memories play an important role in an individual’s decision-making during risk situations. Though decision-making is often assumed to rely more on the recollection of prior experiences, consideration should be given to the impacts of an individual’s severe weather knowledge. In this study, participants were faced with a decision-making task both before and after viewing a 5-minute slide show stimulus of tornadoes and related storm damage. The results indicate a strong correlation between the level of severe weather knowledge and the likelihood to heed the warning associated with the event. Overall, previous tornado experience (HE) indicated a greater tendency to ignore a tornado warning than those who had not experienced a tornado event. The stimulus itself provides tantalizing suggestions about the structure that more effective tornado warnings could take. In this study, the stimulus consisted of tornado footage and related storm damage that may have impacted the warning response among participants. While it has been shown that imagery plays a part in decisionmaking (Leiserowitz 2006), it is not the intent of this paper to determine the impact of the portrayal of tornado images upon the students as an affective component of memory. Generally both D1 and D2 results illustrate that individuals with greater knowledge of severe weather events were less likely to ignore the tornado warning, while those possessing previous tornado experiences were more likely to ignore the warning, especially if they also had a low level of severe weather knowledge. While it is possible that a crossover effect due to the short lapse of time between D1 and D2 may   24   have impacted the likelihood of participants to ignore a tornado warning, it is beyond the scope of this study to determine how separate treatments may have altered the results, and is better determined by further study. Recency of prior tornado warnings may impact the likelihood of action since recent events, even those without an actual tornado event, are more salient in an individual’s memory than a memory of an actual tornado experience in the past. If prior tornado or other severe weather events were relatively minor in intensity, individuals may use this information by anchoring their judgments on information/experiences that are not relevant to their current situation (Newell 2010). Greater knowledge of severe weather provides participants with additional means to determine when precautionary measures should be taken during severe weather. The conclusions reported in this study reinforce the importance of memories on decisionmaking behavior. Specifically, knowledge is a critical factor when one is considering taking precautionary measures during a tornado warning. Modifying an individual’s knowledge, or semantic memory would appear a logical step in influencing precautionary behavior since this could be accomplished through a variety of educational or media avenues. Attempting to modify an individual’s personal experience, or episodic memory would be much more difficult, if not impossible since this would require direct experience with a tornadic event. Since knowledge appears to be a significant factor based on the results of this study, future research will address the limitations of the current study by providing an educational intervention to control for differences in severe weather knowledge among participants.   25   This information, coupled with an understanding of the impacts of additional variables utilized during risk decisions play a significant part in determining future directions in the development of tornado warning practices. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS This work was completed while in residence at the Geocognition Research Laboratory at Michigan State University. I thank Emily Geraghty Ward and Julie Libarkin for assistance with this paper, as well as all students who graciously participated in this research.   26   APPENDICES   27   Appendix 1A: Semantic Questionnaire (SQ1)   28     29   Appendix 1B: Episodic Questionnaire (EQ1)   30   Appendix 1C: Decision-making questionnaire (D1 and D2)   31   Appendix 1D: Statement of Reprint At the time of submission of this dissertation, Chapter 1, “Memory and decision-making: Determining action when the sirens sound” on pages 7 through 32 had been previously published in the Journal of Weather, Climate and Society on January 2013. Drost, Robert, 2013: Memory and decision making: determining action when the sirens sound. Wea. Climate Soc., 5, 43–54. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1175/WCAS-D-11-00042.1 ©American Meteorological Society. Used with permission.   32   REFERENCES   33   REFERENCES Clark, S. K., and Libarkin, J.C., 2011: Designing a mixed-methods research instrument and scoring rubric to investigate individuals’ conceptions of plate tectonics. The Geological Society of America, Special Paper 474. Dillon, R., Tinsley, C., and Cronin, M., 2011: Why Near-Miss Events Can Decrease an Individual’s Protective Response to Hurricanes. Risk Analysis, vol. 31, no. 3. Golafshani, Nahid. 2003: The Qualitative Report. Volume 8, Number 4, p. 597-607. Hertwig, R., Pleskac, T., 2010: Decisions from experience: Why small samples? Cognition, 115, p. 225-237. Johnson, B., 1992: “Advancing Understanding of Knowledge's Role in Lay Risk Perception,” American Sociological Association. Kasperson, R., Kasperson, J., 1996: The Social Amplification and Attenuation of Risk. The Annals of the American Academy, vol. 545. Lazo, J. K., Morss, R. E., and deMuth, J. L., 2009: 300 Billion Served Sources, Perceptions, Uses, and Values of Weather Forecasts. BAMS, p. 785-798. Leiserowitz, A., 2006: Climate Change Risk Perception and Policy Preferences: The role of Affect, Imagery, and Values. Decision Research, 1201 Oak Street, Suite 200, Eugene, OR 97401. Lyerly, et. al., 2007: Risks, Values, and Decision Making Surrounding Pregnancy. Obstetrics & Gynecology, vol. 109, no. 4. Marx, S. M., Weber, E. U., Orlove, B. S., Leiserowitz, A., Krantz, D. H., Roncoli, C., and Phillips, J., 2007: Communication and mental processes: Experiential and analytic processing of uncertain climate information. Science Direct, Global Environmental Change 17, p. 47–58. Morrow, B., 2009: Risk behavior and risk communication: Synthesis and expert interviews final report for the NOAA Coastal Services Center. SocResearch. Morss, et al., 2008: Communicating Uncertainty in Weather Forecasts: A Survey of the U.S. Public. Weather and Forecasting, vol. 23, February, 974-991. National Oceanic and Atmospheric Administration (NOAA), 2010: The official National Weather Service Mission Statement. www.noaa.gov.   34   National Oceanic and Atmospheric Administration (NOAA), 1995: Tornadoes…Nature’s Most Violent Storms. A Preparedness Guide, including Safety Information for Schools. National Oceanic and Atmospheric Administration (NOAA), National Weather Service, 2009: National Centers for Environmental Prediction, Storm Prediction Center. National Weather Service (NWS) Southern Region, 1995: JetStream – An Online School for Weather. www.srh.noaa.gov. Newell, B.R, Pitman A. J., 2010: The Psychology of Global Warming. BAMS, p. 10031014. Riabacke, A., 2006: IAENG International Journal of Computer Science, 32:4. Slovic, P., 2000: The Perception of Risk. Earthscan Publications Ltd. Tulving E., 1972: Organization of memory: Episodic and semantic memory. Academic Press, p. 381-403. UKCIP Technical Report 2003: Climate adaptation: Risk, uncertainty and decisionmaking. Whitmarsh, L., 2008: Are flood victims more concerned about climate change than other people? The role of direct experience in risk perception and behavioral response. Journal of Risk Research, vol. 11, no. 3, p. 351–374.   35   Chapter 2 GSA members on climate change: Where, what, and ways forward? 2.1 Introduction Climate change is one of the most pressing environmental, economic, and societal issues of the 21st century. Addressing climate change issues is difficult partly due to the disconnect between the scientific community and the public’s understanding and perception of climate change issues. A number of studies have examined the views of the public and suggest that basic knowledge about climate change is limited and that many believe there is still no agreement in the scientific community about the possible causes and impacts (Hamilton, 2011; Whitmarsh, 2009). Fewer studies have looked at the broad scientific community, but those that have report that climate scientists who understand the climate process generally accept that anthropogenic climate change exists and agree that human activity has had a profound impact on Earth’s climate (Doran, 2009; Oreskes, 2004). However, in order for vital information to be passed onto the policy makers and voters, the scientists must put forth the effort to inform. Included in that effort is the need to communicate the agreement scientist’s share on climate change factors. Studies indicate that only 47% of the American public believes that there is scientific consensus on climate change (Doran, 2009); this belief needs to be addressed if scientist’s views are to be accepted by the public. Here we look at the perceptions of Geological Society of America (GSA) scientists on climate change to understand: what are their biggest concerns, what regions of the USA will be most impacted, and how do we bridge the gap between scientists and the public?   36   2.2 Methods Surveys were collected in the exhibit halls of the GSA Annual Meetings in 2009 and 2011. GSA has over 20,000 members and each annual meeting attracts nearly 6,000 geoscientists (www.geosociety.org). We collected 181 surveys. Forty-nine percent of participants were female, and ages ranged from 19 to 70 years. The brief two-page survey varied slightly between the two years but both asked participants to indicate on a map where they believe climate change will have the most impact. They were then asked to answer questions (seen in Table 2.1) about the region they indicated. Table 2.1: Survey questions and response information. Please contact authors for access to full survey results. Question Response Type Shade in one region on the map below that you feel has been or will Shading/Circling be impacted by climate change. This region has been or will be severely impacted by climate change. Likert Scale I believe the general public is sufficiently informed about the impacts Likert Scale of climate change in this region. Describe the climate change impact(s) that the region you shaded Open-ended has or will experience. Explain what you believe would be the most effective way to increase Open-ended public understanding of climate change. 2.3 Results The most common response to the open-ended question about the impact of climate change was sea level rise (32%) followed by more severe weather (22%) and water resource issues (20%). These responses make sense since people generally show the most concern for “salient, palatable” risks (Seacrest, 2000). The remaining responses were varied and specific with the next most common being agricultural shifts, both spatially and temporally (6%). Geographically specific impacts included pine-beetle expansion and loss of the maple-syrup industry. In all 181 surveys, there were only two   37   “climate change skeptics” that clearly stated they do not believe anything will occur because climate change is not happening. Circled regions from the surveys were digitized into ESRI ArcGIS to visualize the overall regions of concern. Figure 2.1 represents the overall density of regions circled across all participants. The focus on coasts is consistent with the impacts given on the open-ended portion of the survey, including sea level rise and and increase of hurricanes. Water resource issues are also reflected in the focus on the southwestern United States. There were minor differences between years based on the location of the meeting (more focus on the northwest in 2009 and the northern midwest in 2011). These signals, however, were relatively insignificant compared to the concern over the coasts. Figure 2.1: Image showing the regions of most climate change concerns among GSA respondents. Darker orange areas are where more respondents shaded in. 2.4 Discussion and Conclusions Survey results indicate that 89 percent of the respondents believe that climate change presents a significant risk to the public, whereas only about half the general population is concerned. This difference in perceived risk may be influenced by a number of potentially mitigating factors shared by both the respondents and the public.   38   These include personal experiences with climate change (Whitmarsh, 2009) as well as social and demographic factors (Leiserowitz, 2006). Perhaps the difference is rooted in the scientist’s understanding and acceptance of the evidence, which may be immune to influences by informational sources available to the public. Although recent studies have demonstrated varying beliefs in climate change by Americans (Hamilton, 2011; Whitmarsh, 2009), substantial doubt and lower perceived risk of climate change still remain among the population. As the public’s trusted source, what do these scientists think of the current state of the public’s awareness of climate change? Most respondents (84%) believe the public is not adequately informed on the potential climate change impacts in the United States. This position is reflected by recent studies that indicate the American public is not well informed on climate change issues (Malka et al., 2009). Although potentially alarming, the geoscientists were forthcoming with possible solutions to increasing public awareness. The majority of survey respondents (52%) believe the public is best informed through educational means, varying from formal K-12 education to specific public outreach programs delivered through a variety of methods to enable the greatest coverage. The remainder of responses varied in the delivery mechanism of climate information to the public. Some believed in a pure source of information derived from the scientific community, while others felt the government should take a role in disseminating the information in an understandable public format. Interestingly, 6% of the respondents indicated that an actual climate related disaster would serve best to wake up the public to the risk associated with climate change in their respective region.   39   Although drastic in comparison with more reasonable alternatives, the impact of disasters and national attention focused on these events has the tendency to grip the public’s scrutiny in an immediate and urgent manner. The link between scientists and the public thirst for knowledge is an opportunity for the geoscience community. The public generally relies on the media to navigate science-based issues, ranging from local weather to complex information about geo-happenings, including climate change. Since scientists usually generate this information, a more direct connection between the media and scientists, or perhaps alternative methods of providing for the interaction between scientists and the public would be beneficial. Malka et al. (2009) show that nearly three-quarters of the public relies on scientists for information because the complexity and number of issues is too much to fully grasp without conducting research oneself. Survey data demonstrates that climate change concerns among geoscientists are consistent and aligned with current climate science. This community has great potential to influence public awareness and understanding of climate issues by acting in unison (Anderegg et al., 2010) and reinforcing the public’s trust (Hamilton, 2011; Whitmarsh 2009). The GSA’s official position statement on climate change highlights the opportunities available to members in order to help this cause. These include participating in professional education, engaging in public education activities, collaborating with stakeholders, working with other science and policy societies, and utilizing the most up-to-date sources of climate science (GSA 2010). The impacts of climate change range from local communities to the global population. With overwhelming consensus, and armed with the best science, each member of the   40   geoscience community can find their niche in moving the public toward better understanding of the risks and solutions for the changing climate.   41   APPENDIX   42   Appendix 2A: Statement of Reprint At the time of submission of this dissertation, Chapter 2, “GSA members on climate change: Where, what, and ways forward?” on pages 32 through 39 had been previously published in the Geological Society of America’s publication, GSA Today on January 2013. Drost, Robert E., and Sheldon P. Turner. "GSA members on climate change: Where, what, and ways forward?" GSA Today 23.1 (2013). doi: 10.1130/GSATG157GW.1 ©Geological Society of America. Used with permission.   43   REFERENCES   44   REFERENCES William R. L. Anderegg, James W. Prall, Jacob Harold, and Stephen H. Schneider, 2010. Expert credibility in climate change. PNAS 107(27) 12107–12109. Ariel Malka, Jon A. Krosnick, and Gary Langer, 2009. “The Association of Knowledge with Concern about Global Warming: Trusted Information Sources Shape Public Thinking.” Risk Analysis 29, 633-647. Bray, D., von Storch, H., 2010. A Survey of the Perspectives of Climate Scientists Concerning Climate Science and Climate Change. GKSS 2010/9. Doran, P. T., 2009. Examining the Scientific Consensus on Climate Change. EOS, Vol. 90, No. 3. Geological Society of America. 2010. “GSA Position Statement - Climate Change”. Retrieved from http://www.geosociety.org/positions/position10.htm Hamilton, L., 2011. Climate Change Partisanship, Understanding, and Public Opinion, Casey Institute Issue brief No. 26. Leiserowitz, A., 2006. Climate Change Risk Perception and Policy Preferences: The role of Affect, Imagery, and Values. Decision Research, Eugene, OR. Oreskes, N., 2004. Beyond the ivory tower: The scientific consensus on climate change. Science, 306 (5702), 1686. Seacrest, S., Kuzelka, R., Leonard, R., 2000. Global climate change and public perception: The challenge of translation. Journal of the American Water Resources Association. 36(2), 253-263. Whitmarsh, L., 2009. What’s in a name? Commonalities and differences in public understanding of “climate change” and “global warming”, Public Understanding of Science, 18, 401–420.   45   Chapter 3 Gesturing During Weathercasts: A “hands down” Best Practice? Televised media is one of the most frequently accessed sources of weather information. The local weathercaster serves as the vital link between the viewing public and weather data and associated forecast models. As such, weathercaster characteristics, from vocal cadence to physical appearance, can impact viewer understanding. This study considers the role of weathercaster gesturing on viewer attention during weather forecasts. Two variations of a typical weather forecast were viewed by a total of 36 participants during an eye tracking experiment. The first forecast variation contained physical gestures towards forecast text by the newscaster (Gesture condition), while the second variation contained minimal gesturing (No Gesture condition). Following each eye tracking experiment, participants completed a retention survey related to the forecast. The resulting data were used to identify specific areas of interest that participants attended to during viewing and to ascertain how well the forecast was retained across both conditions. Study results suggest that the weathercaster’s gesturing during forecasts impacted viewer attention, but did not affect retention of weather information. Gesturing diverted attention from other areas of interest within the forecast by encouraging participants to focus on the weathercaster’s hands. This study indicates that minor modifications to weathercaster behavior can produce significant changes in viewer behavior. In practice, it appears acceptable to gesture in situations where there are few areas that may divert viewer attention from salient to non-salient information. However, in situations where viewer attention needs to be focused on salient information, gesturing is not recommended.   46   3.1 Introduction Recent studies indicate the general population accesses weather information through personal observation, newspapers, radio, television, and the internet (Lazo, et. al. 2008 and Drost 2013). Additional work indicates that television is still the most popular source for weather, traffic, and breaking news despite the availability of many other media outlets (Pew Research 2011, Harris 2007 and Lazo 2002). As a source of this information, network or cable television is accessed almost daily in most households, particularly for newscasts or weather (Lazo, et. al. 2008). As a consequence, the local or national weathercaster plays a vital role in disseminating weather, climate, and even general science to the general public (Morrow 2008 and NRC 2003). With almost 3,000 television stations in the United States (ERI 2011), the delivery and style of weather forecasts is also highly variable. Effective transfer of information during weather forecasts may be impacted by a variety of factors such as how text, animations, pictures and color are used within the forecast (Morss 2008). Forecasts may utilize text, visuals, or animations to deliver information, although most rely on the weathercaster speaking directly to the audience (Trobec 2007). The forecast itself may incorporate technical terms, employ humor, or use knowledge of local places to relay weather information (Socci 2007). In addition to the actual delivery of forecast information, elements such as station logos, live video footage and informational text scrolls may also be displayed to viewers (WMO 2005). These varying elements and styles of delivery may all potentially impact the viewing audience’s perception and retention of weather forecast information (Stewart 2006). Previous research has utilized eye-tracking studies to ascertain the roles of   47   these elements and their impact on viewers learning and retention (Stark et. al. 2007) and to determine the impacts to comprehension when combining conventional text with a variety of reinforcing imagery (Yaros & Cook 2011). These studies (Stark et. al. 2007 and Yaros & Cook 2011) confirm that visual representations tend to reinforce viewing time and fixation on salient features when information is presented both visually and in text. In addition, individuals spend more time on specific information being presented when reinforced through the use of gesturing (Louwerse and Bangerter 2000). . This recording of human eye movements with eye-tracking software provides insight into aspects of fields of view that attract (or distract) viewer attention. Eye movement data captures the punctuated progression of eyes across stimuli in the field of view. Eye movements dart from feature to feature in jerky actions called saccadic movements (Liversedge and Findlay 2000) that ultimately fall in the intervals between fixations. A fixation (Table 3.1) occurs when eye movement pauses on a specific area of the visual field for more than a defined length of time (Tobii 2010), suggesting attention to an underlying feature. This attention can be inferred to relate to cognitive processes (Just and Carpenter 1976). In general, fixation data can be used to infer which aspects of stimuli attract attention, while other characteristics of eye tracking data provide supporting evidence for interpreting cognitive mechanisms or rationales (Table 3.1). Of particular interest are areas of interest (AOI), established based on the content of the forecast and the weathercaster’s on-screen position depending on which condition was being viewed. AOI may also be determined by evaluating the number of fixations on a certain area   48   such as a graphic element, and may be indicative of a participant’s interest in that element. Fixation duration may also be used to determine important AOI, providing evidence for which stimuli are most attractive to users, or possibly most confusing (Waterworth et. al. 2003). Fixation count represents the actual number of fixations within an AOI and often impacts other fixation data in the form of higher measured results. In the form of a graphical output, gaze plots can be used to determine the movements and positions where the participant was looking during the entire eye tracking session, while heat maps provide for a visual representation using cool to warm colors (typically green to red) that indicate how long or how often the participants fixated on an image. Characteristics of gaze plots such as total path length, time spent on fixations within an AOI, average fixation time, and the number of occurrences in which a viewer crossed their own gaze path, are quantifiable and thus make viewer attention on an image or video comparable. From this data we can ascertain which AOI were viewed first, which attracted or deflected attention, and the length of time over which each AOI was viewed. Heat maps representing the visual generalization of gaze data can be useful in qualitatively identifying which image or scene commanded viewer attention. Areas of most intense focus are normally represented in red. Apparent heat map intensities can be visually compared, but resulting qualitative metrics are biased since the analysis of the patterns is often based on the individual interpretation and should be replaced using quantitative metrics when possible (Ehmke & Wilson 2007). This research focused on participant interest and retention of specific forecast elements (AOI) utilizing gaze data collected during an eye tracking session. The data   49   were used to identify the link between where and how long a participant looked at the forecast video and how these were impacted by weathercaster gesturing. The data were then used to determine whether gesturing impacted participant retention of the forecast elements contained within the AOI. The current study considered the relationship between viewer attention and information retention in response to actual weather forecast communication and investigated the role of forecaster gesturing on viewer retention. The study builds upon current research by integrating the outcomes related to the variety of factors that may affect forecast transference and the impact of gesturing on viewer attention and retention. This study will attempt to answer the following research questions: 1) Does gesturing by the weathercaster increase viewer attention to forecast elements? 2) Does weathercaster gesturing cause viewers to redirect their attention to particular forecast elements 2) Does gesturing impact viewer attention and retention of critical forecast information? 3) How does gesturing, or lack of gesturing, impact viewer attention on forecast elements? Table 3.1: Definitions of gaze data used in this study. Term Areas of Interest Fixation Fixation Duration Data Use as interpreted by researcher Identify participant interest An element of interest contained in specific forecast within the forecast clip. elements. Also known as gaze cluster centroid: Identify visual attention on a single location characterizing the features within the field of geographic center of a cluster of data view The length of time a participant spent fixated on a specific feature. Determine visual attention hierarchy Calculated as the sum of fixations within a specific feature. Study definition     50   Table 3.1 (cont’d) Fixation Count Gaze plots Heat maps Number of fixations per feature. Indication of pattern of eye movement around an image, including intervals of no movement and time spent looking at specific areas Spatial map of areas with longest total gaze Identify visual attention on features within the field of view Ascertain elements viewed first, elements that attracted or deflected attention, and image comprehension. Indicate elements that are particularly attractive. 3.2 Methods Thirty-six undergraduate students at a large Midwestern institution participated in the experiment. The mean age of the experimental population was 20 years (SD=1.056) and was composed of male (n=10) and female (n=26) participants. 75% of the population was Caucasian, 14% Asian, 8% African American. Participants were nonscience majors with and were paid $25 for their time. 3.2.1 Data Collection Data were collected in three successive steps during the study. Participants first completed a survey consisting of demographic questions related to age, gender, ethnicity, and geoscience experience. Participants were given a brief explanation of the eye tracking equipment and its function before completing a consent form and engaging in the experiment. The study was conducted as part of an hour-long eye tracking experiment that consisted of multiple studies investigating participant’s familiarities and opinions regarding climate change and geosciences. During this particular experiment, participants viewed one of two versions of a nearly identical weekend weather forecast of approximately 26 seconds in length and were told to watch as if observing an actual   51   upcoming weekend forecast. In the No Gesture condition (Figure 3.1 - A), the weathercaster is relatively motionless and speaking directly at the audience. In the Gesture condition (Figure 3.1 - B), the weathercaster is seen gesturing with his hands toward the on-screen weather forecast text. The Gesture condition consisted of 17 participants and the No Gesture condition consisted of 19 participants. A Tobii T60 Eye Tracker was used to collect eye position, time, and validity data every 1/60th of a second during the experiment. The Tobii T60 determines eye position using multiple techniques including both bright and dark pupil tracking (Tobii 2011). Sixty gaze data points were collected for each eye every second. Resulting gaze calculations were determined by Tobii Studio 3.0 software designed for the eye-tracker (Tobii 2010). The eye tracker captured viewer visual attention as they interacted with the forecast. Immediately following the eye tracking session, participants completed a retention questionnaire used to determine which elements of the forecast they were able to remember (Appendix 3A). Each eye tracking and questionnaire session ended with a brief explanation of the purpose of the experiments and an opportunity to ask any general questions. Figure 3.1: A - No Gesture condition forecast variation and B - Gesture condition forecast variation. Both forecasts were identical except for the presence or absence of the weathercaster’s gesturing. Forecasts provided by KELO-TV, Jay Trobec, Chief Meteorologist.   52   For the purpose of this study, seven Areas of Interest (AOI; Table 3.2), within the forecast were identified: Station Banner, Days of the Week, Scene Image, Forecast, Forecast Temperature, Weathercaster’s Hands, and Weathercaster’s Face (Figure 3.2 A and 3.2 - B; Table 3.2). Eye tracking data related to each AOI were used to understand how interactions with each AOI change in the presence or absence of gesture. ArcGIS, a mapping and spatial analysis software suite commonly applied to geographic data (ESRI 2001), was used to link AOI with eye tracking data. Each AOI was digitized using the common mapping tool in ArcGIS to create polygon and shapefiles that represent each AOI. Each AOI shapefile was then overlain onto a frozen frame of both forecast videos (Figure 3.2 - A and 3.2 - B). In this particular case, the videos were relatively static since the weathercaster remained mostly motionless other than the specific hand and limited head movement. This relative static positioning allowed us to identify accurate AOI across the forecast video. After viewing the video, participants completed a questionnaire assessing the extent to which forecast information was retained (Appendix 3A). The questionnaire contained seven questions, five related to weather and two related to the weathercaster’s attire and screen background. Eye tracking data provided location, duration, and the gaze track of the participants’ visual attention during the videos, while the questionnaire data provided: 1) participants’ recollection of the weekend weather forecast; 2) participants’ recollection of non-forecast information (shirt color and background color).   53   3.2.2 Data Analysis Participant questionnaire responses were collected and analyzed quantitatively (justification of methods are found in Appendix 3B). Eye tracking data was then imported into ArcGIS and filtered using custom Python scripts. Raw eye tracking data was filtered to remove invalid data. This can be defined as a poor validity measurement for one or both eyes during instances of blinking or when the eye tracker cannot constrain the eye position effectively. Olsson’s (2007) interpolation method was used to interpolate gaze for intervals where invalid data was removed. The filtered gaze data was then grouped into fixations to eliminate data points that represent instances when the participants were not focusing their gaze. We chose to use Salvucci & Goldberg’s (2000) fixation filter. This filter is based on spatial dispersion of the eye tracking data and defines a cluster and being a set of sequential eye tracking points that are close in spatial proximity. We used the same distance threshold of 35 pixels for clustering as per Tobii Studio eye tracking software default. The minimum fixation time is generally accepted by visual scientists to be 80ms (White 2008). Our eye tracking data were collected every 1/60th of a second, requiring 5 sequential points to exceed 80 ms. Each fixation has a geographic average location, known as the fixation or gaze. The number of gaze cluster centroids and consequently the time spent within each AOI was then summarized using ArcGIS’s Select by Location querying tool for each forecast variation. Demographic and retention surveys were analyzed via simple descriptive statistics and included a skewness and kurtosis analysis to determine the normality of the study population. An independent samples T-test was performed to determine if gender impacted retention questionnaire results; age and ethnicity of the study   54   population varied only slightly and were not considered as covariates. The total number of fixations, average fixation duration, and total fixation duration were compared for both the No Gesture and Gesture conditions in order to determine differences in the amount of time each group spent fixated on each of the seven AOI. A Mann-Whitney U Test was performed on the two conditions, No Gesture and Gesture, in order to determine which had greater participant fixation and fixation duration for each AOI. This particular test is useful when investigating whether or not a difference exists between two groups. In this case, the two groups would be the No Gesture and Gesture forecast variations. An independent samples T-test was performed to determine differences in retention scores among the No Gesture and Gesture groups in order to ascertain the impact of gesturing during each condition. Lastly, gaze plots and heat maps were generated using ArcGIS, in order to identify patterns of eye movement and determine elements of each condition found attractive by the study participants. These elements, known as AOI, were initially identified prior to data collection based on traditional elements contained in a weather forecast. Once data collection had been completed the information was used to determine actual AOI location and boundaries based on the eye gaze of the participants. Raw eye track data exported from Tobii Studio were converted into ArcGIS-compatible formats using a custom ArcGIS tool written in Python. The tool iteratively clusters the raw eye tracking data and removes eye position measurements characterized by low confidence based on the validity code (Tobii 2011). Gaze cluster centroids representing fixations were connected using ArcGIS10.0’s Points to Line tool to make gaze plots for each participant (Figure 3.3A, and 3.3B). Gaze plots   55   represent the path of viewer attention across an image and help to characterize both saccadic and smooth movements. Figure 3.2: A - No Gesture condition with associated AOI and B - Gesture condition with associated AOI. All but one AOI are identical; the Hands AOI varies based on main gesturing position in the video. Table 3.2: AOI letter designation and corresponding areas in pixels (units squared). *Areas are identical except for the Weathercaster Hand AOI. AOI Name Letter Designation No gesture pixels (units2) Gesture pixels (units 2) Station Banner Days of the Week Scene Image Forecast Forecast Temperature Weathercaster’s Hands* Weathercaster’s Face A B C D E F G 22220.9 6639.3 22166.0 76787.4 6852.2 22937.1 10064.5 22220.9 6639.3 22166.0 76787.4 6852.2 26167.8 10064.5 3.3 Results The results of this study include descriptive data of the study population and findings based on retention and gaze data collected during the study. Retention data represents the participant’s correct responses to questions regarding forecast elements and AOI data indicates where and how long participants attended to particular forecast elements. As a result, both the retention and gaze data may be indicative of the   56   attention paid to the forecast by the participants. Descriptive data indicated that the study population was normal based on the results of the skewness and kurtosis results in which statistical values for gender, age and ethnicity all fell within an acceptable statistical range of -2 to 2. Retention survey results indicate that no difference exists between No Gesture and Gesture forecast variations when comparing the participants’ correct responses to a total of seven questions that focused on important elements of forecast and nonforecast elements related to the scene itself. An independent samples T-test was conducted to compare retention scores for the No Gesture and Gesture group. No significant difference in the scores exists between the No Gesture (M = 2.63, SD = 1.17) and the Gesture groupings for forecast scores (M = 2.94, SD = 1.29); t(34) = -.770, p = 0.447. No significant difference was found for the non-forecast scores with the No Gesture (M = 1.16, SD = .688) and the Gesture groupings (M = 1.18, SD = .728); t(34) = -.079, p = .938. These results suggest that weathercaster gesturing had no effect on participant retention of forecast information. In order to ascertain if gender differences might impact retention of the weather forecast information during the No Gesture and Gesture conditions an independent samples T-test was performed. The results indicated that gender was not a significant factor in scores for female (M=2.84, SD=1.15) and male (M= 2.70, SD= 1.41) conditions in this study; t(34) = 3.19, p = .752. No additional tests were performed on the remaining demographic data (age and ethnicity) since these data had minimal variation. Eye tracking data were used to determine the total number of fixations, average fixation duration, and total fixation duration spent on each AOI across the study   57   population (Table 3.3). The total number of fixations were greater for the No Gesture condition AOI compared to the gesture condition AOI, except for the Weathercaster’s Hands and Face AOIs. The average fixation duration for the No Gesture condition AOI were also greater than the Gesture condition AOI, except for a slightly higher duration in the Weathercaster’s hand AOI for the Gesture condition. The total fixation duration for the No Gesture condition AOI were greater than the Gesture condition AOI for all AOI. For both conditions, the Forecast temperature, Forecast and Weathercaster’s face AOI induced the greatest gaze among participants, while the Station banner, Days of the week, Scene image and Weathercaster’s hands AOI induced the least amount of gaze. AOI for both conditions were similar except for the Hands AOI, which represented a larger area (pixels) due to the movement of the weathercaster’s hands during the video. Table 3.3: Total fixations, average fixation duration and total fixation duration for both No gesture and Gesture conditions. No Gesture n=19, Gesture n=17. Area of Interest (AOI) Total number of fixations No Gesture Gesture Average fixation duration No Gesture Gesture Total fixation duration No Gesture Gesture Station banner Days of the week Scene image Forecast temperature Forecast 116 115 161 262 76 93 105 242 .44 .66 .53 .54 .18 .26 .23 .25 8.4 12.55 10.16 10.34 3.04 4.39 3.83 4.24 1134 986 .54 .24 10.19 4.07 Weathercaster’s hands Weathercaster’s face 11 39 .18 .19 3.43 3.22 167 177 .96 .36 18.18 6.1 The results of the Mann-Whitney U indicate that in most comparisons, eye tracking data were similar across the two conditions where Z = 1.96, p <= .05. The only   58   exceptions to this finding were U scores associated with the “Centroids Hands Percent”, U = 81.5, p =.006 and the “Fixation Duration Hands”, U = 97, p = .029 (Table 3.4). Fixation   Duration  Face   Fixation   Duration  Hands   Fixation   Duration  Scene   Fixation   Duration   Temperature   81.5   108   147   115   108   139   107.5   97   117.5   117.5   298.5   -­‐0.514   0.607   282   -­‐1.041   0.298   325.5   -­‐0.827   0.408   271.5   -­‐2.723   0.006   261   -­‐1.709   0.088   337   -­‐0.481   0.645   268   -­‐1.48   0.130   261   -­‐1.067   0.09   292   -­‐0.710   0.472   260.5   -­‐1.712   0.087   287   -­‐2.186   0.029   270.5   -­‐1.307   0.182   270.5   -­‐1.307   0.162   b .616   b .315   b .415   b .010   b .003   b .661   b .145   b .003   Fixation   Duration   Station  Banner   Centroids   Temperature   Percent   135.5   Fixation   Duration  Days   of  the  Week   Centroids   Scene  Percent   129   Fixation   Duration   Forecast   Centroids   Hands  Percent   145.5   Centroids   Station  Banner   Percent   Centroids  Face   Percent   Mann-­‐ 157   Whitney  U   Wilcoxon  W   347   Z   -­‐0.143   Asymp.  Sig.   0.888   (2-­‐tailed)   b Exact  Sig.   .900   [2+(1-­‐tailed   Sig.)]   a.  Grouping  variable:  Trial   b.  Not  corrected  for  ties.   Centroids  Days   of  the  Week   Percent   Centroids   Forecast   Percent   Test  Statistics   Table 3.4: Results of the Mann-Whitney U Test Statistics for No Gesture and Gesture conditions. b .490   b .067   b .042   b .185   b .185   Finally, gaze plots and heat maps were generated using ArcGIS. These were used to illustrate gaze patterns, fixation duration and forecast elements particularly attractive to participants. The gaze plots are useful in attempting to understand what elements were of particular interest to the participant and how they moved their attention from one element to another. The heat maps are useful in determining which elements were found attractive by the participant, potentially drawing attention not only to themselves, but also away from other elements. The gaze plot (Figure 3.3A and 3.3B) depicts one participant’s complete gaze path. Circles represent each fixation; the size of the circle reflects the duration of fixation where larger circles indicate longer fixation times. The lines between fixations indicate the pattern of eye movement by the participant. In the No Gesture gaze plot, the participant’s attention appears to be heavily concentrated on the weathercaster’s face and the forecast. Other areas of participant attention, though somewhat less, are the scene image, days of the week and station   59   banner. In addition to the heavy concentration, the participant also demonstrates a pattern of uninterrupted movement between the weathercaster’s face and the forecast. There is no apparent attention given to the weathercaster’s body or hands. Similar to the No Gesture gaze plot, the Gesture gaze plot also demonstrates areas of heavily concentrated attention on the weathercaster’s face and the forecast. However, the plot appears to show the participant moving from the weathercaster’s face, stopping in the area close to the hands and then moving to the forecast. There also appears to be a greater amount of attention focused on the days of the week than illustrated in the No Gesture plot. Figure 3.3: A and B are example gaze plots during No Gesture condition and Gesture condition forecast variations. Each depiction illustrates one participant’s gaze during each forecast. In Figure 3 A is a gaze plot for Participant Elab2011020a during the No Gesture condition forecast variation and B shows a gaze plot for Participant Elab2011006b during the Gesture condition forecast variation. Heat maps (Figure 3.4) are a eye tracking visualization tool that display a summary of gaze data across multiple participants. In this study, red on a heat map indicates the general location where participant gaze data focused most often, green indicates an area of lesser focus, and no color indicates no data was recorded for these   60   areas. Areas of high fixation represent potential areas of interest to the participant because participants spent more time overall visually attending to these areas. The No Gesture heat map indicates higher concentration of participant’s attention on the weathercaster’s face and forecast. There also appears to be moderate levels of attention on the days of the week. The scene image and station banner indicate a lesser amount of participant attention being paid to these elements. The Gesture heat map generated results similar to the No Gesture except for a lesser amount of attention being focused on the weathercaster’s face. There does appear to be a more pronounced connection between the weathercaster’s face and the forecast and days of the week, however, when compared to the No Gesture heat map. Figure 3.4: Example heat maps for the No Gesture condition (A) and Gesture condition (B) forecast variations. Each depiction illustrates the study population’s gaze during each forecast. In Figure 3.4 A is a heat map for study participants during the No Gesture condition forecast variation and B is a heat map for study participants during the Gesture condition forecast variation. 3.4 Discussion and Conclusions This study considered the extent to which weathercaster gesturing would impact viewer attention and retention during a televised weather forecast. After viewing one of two video clips (No Gesture or Gesture) of an actual weather forecast, participants were   61   asked seven questions directly related to the forecast they previously viewed. Five of the questions were related to actual forecast elements and the remaining two pertained to the weathercaster shirt color and background scene color. Quantitative and statistical analysis indicated no difference in the retention scores for both the No Gesture and Gesture conditions. A closer look at the fixation data (Table 3.3) however reveals some interesting findings that appear to support gesturing influencing participant attention during the video clips. Higher total fixations were recorded for all AOI during the No Gesture condition except for the hands AOI and face AOI. Total fixations for the hands and face AOI for the Gesture condition were greater than those recorded for the No Gesture condition. It is possible that the participant’s attention was distracted by the hand movement, causing them to focus on the hands more and reducing attention on other elements of the video. Except for the hands AOI, average fixation duration is lower for all other elements for the Gesture condition. Previous research indicates that extended fixation duration may be an indicator of confusion, causing the participant to spend additional time trying to understand what is being presented. As a result, it may be that participants viewing the No Gesture condition were somewhat confused by the forecast and required more time to process the information being presented. Total fixation duration was higher for the No Gesture condition compared to the Gesture condition, but retention scores were not significantly different. This may be attributable to participant confusion experienced during the No gesture condition due to   62   the lack of weathercaster gesturing, resulting in additional time spent to understand the forecast. The most significant differences between both gesture trials were the number of fixations and the length of time of those fixations on the weathercaster’s face (Face AOI) and hands (Hands AOI). In particular, the hands gesture encouraged the greatest difference in viewer attention diversion from the forecast in terms of fixation duration, between both gesture trials. Gesturing diverts viewer attention from particular points of interest contained in the weather forecast without negatively impacting viewer retention of forecast information. Being aware of the impacts of gesturing, weathercasters may be able to alter viewer behavior using gesturing to aid in transitions associated with traditional forecasts by altering the viewers focus on specific elements. It appears that for the purpose of this study that gesturing by the weathercaster distracts undergraduate participants from the time spent on important weekend weather information during typical weather forecasts. In addition, participant interaction with on screen graphics and text may combine with gesturing to alter attention to the various elements contained within the forecast. While no gain in retention was realized by forecasts in which gesturing by the weathercaster is prominent (Gesture condition), the increased fixation on the weathercaster’s hands (Hands AOI), and diminished fixation on other AOI included in the video, as well the potential impact of graphic elements (station banner), should be considered by the weathercaster in determining best practices when developing appealing weather forecasts. Careful analysis of on-air forecast elements, weathercaster position and subsequent gesturing techniques all impact viewer attention. Weather forecast elements, weathercaster position and   63   movements all have the capacity to influence viewer retention and attention of the material being presented. Specific attention should be paid to elements that may cause confusion, or increased concentration on the part of the viewer, during weather forecasts. While gesturing has not been shown to impact retention of the material being presented, it does have the ability to redirect attention during forecasts. The redirection may be responsible for reducing confusion or concentration necessary for the viewer to understand forecast elements. Since television continues to be a popular source for newsworthy information, careful thought and consideration should be given to the design elements and posturing of the on screen personality. This is especially important in cases where the weathercaster is considered the “science link” to the general public, often being the case in local and large market televised media. In situations where minimal elements are competing for viewer attention, gesturing may be appropriate since attention will not be diverted from salient information. However, in instances where gesturing would cause viewer attention to be diverted from salient information during the forecast, gesturing is not recommended. This applies to not only weather related segments, but the entire newscast as a whole. Maximizing the viewers experience by reducing confusion and unnecessary concentration on non-salient elements should lead to increased attention to the information being provided and insure ample retention. This study supports the future need for additional investigation into weathercaster delivery methods and broadcast elements and their impact on viewer attention and retention. The information presented during these broadcasts often contain critical information pertaining to not only weather related events, but other major happenings   64   requiring decisions to be made by viewers. Communication that is accurate and easily comprehended without distraction provides the basis for timely consideration of the material being presented, allowing for purposeful decision-making.   65   APPENDICES   66   Appendix 3A: Retention Survey Questions   67   Appendix 3B: Analytical Methods Descriptive statistics were used to explain the results of a demographic survey consisting of questions related to age, gender, ethnicity, and geoscience experience. Descriptive statistics are used to describe the base level data of a study and provide simple summaries. They are typically used as a base level of study information and provide a basis for further quantitative analysis of the study. For this study descriptive statistics were used to determine whether the study population was normal based on a the results of the skewness and kurtosis results in which statistical values for gender, age and ethnicity all fell within an acceptable statistical range of -2 to 2. An Independent samples T-test was performed to compare retention of the forecast and non-forecast information presented in the clip. Independent samples T-tests are used to compare the means of two independent samples, or populations. It is often utilized to test a hypothesis based on the difference between two different groups (samples). For this study the retention scores of both No Gesture and Gesture groups were compared to determine if statistical differences existed between the two groups. An additional independent samples T test was performed to investigate the impact gender had on retention of forecast information between male and female participants. A Mann-Whitney U test was used to compare eye tracking data of AOI identified by the study. The data was used to determine differences in participant attention to different forecast elements contained within each clip (No Gesture and Gesture). The Mann-Whitney U-test is used to determine whether two independent samples of observations are drawn from the same distributions of collected data. An advantage of the test is that the two separate samples do not have to contain the same number of observations. In this case the number of fixations were compared between the No Gesture and Gesture groups for the AOI identified by the study to determine if gesturing served to direct a participant’s attention to a particular AOI.   68   REFERENCES   69   REFERENCES Drost, R. (2013): Memory and decision-making: Determining action when the sirens sound. Weather, Climate and Society. Electronic Research Incorporated ERI 2011. Ehmke, C., Wilson, S. (2007): Identifying Web Usability Problems from Eye-Tracking Data. Published by the British Computer Society People and Computers XXI – HCI... but not as we know it: Proceedings of HCI 2007. ESRI (2001): ArcGIS TM Spatial Analyst: Advanced GIS Spatial Analysis Using Raster and Vector Data. ESRI White paper J8747. Harris, (2007): Local television news is the place for weather forecasts for a plurality of Americans. The Harris Poll #118, November 28, 2007. Available online at (http://www.harrisinteractive.com/harris_poll/index.asp?PID=839). Just, M., A., Carpenter, P., A. (1976): Eye Fixations and Cognitive Processes. Cognitive Psychology. Volume 8, 441-480. Liversedge, S., P., Findlay, J., M. (2000): Saccadic eye movements and cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. Volume 4, No.1. Lazo, J., K., and Chestnut, L., G. (2002): Economic value of current and improved weather forecasts in the U.S. household sector. Report to the NOAA Office of Policy and Planning, pp. 213. Lazo, J., K., Morss, R., E., Demuth, J., L. (2008): 300 Billion Served: Sources, Perceptions, Uses, and Values of Weather Forecasts. National Center for Atmospheric Research (NCAR). Louwerse, M., M., Bangerter, A. (2000): Focusing a\Attention with Deictic Gestures and Linguistic Expressions. Institute for Intelligent Systems. 1331-1336. Morrow, B., H., Lazo, J., K., Demuth, J., L. (2008): Communicating Weather Forecast Uncertainty: An Exploratory Study with Broadcast Meteorologists. Final Report of Focus Groups Conducted at the 36th AMS Conference on Broadcast Meteorology Denver, CO June 25-26, 2008. Morss, R., E., Demuth, J., L., Lazo, J., K. (2008): Communicating Uncertainty in Weather Forecasts: A survey of the U.S. Public. American Meteorological Society. Volume 23, pg. 974-991.   70   National Research Council (NRC), (2003): Communicating Uncertainties in Weather and Climate Information: A Workshop Summary. National Acad. Press, 68 pp. Olsson, P. (2007) “Real-time and offline filters for eye tracking,” Master’s thesis, Royal Institute of Technology, Apr. 2007. Pew Research Center for the People & the Press (2011): Internet Gains on Television as Public’s Main News Source. January 4, 2011. Rashbass, C. (1961): The Relationship Between Saccadic and Smooth Tracking Eye Movements, Journal of Physiology, Great Britain. Salvucci, D., Goldberg, J. (2000): Identifying fixations and saccades in eye-tracking protocols. Proceedings of the Eye Tracking Research and Applications Symposium 2000 (pp. 71–78). NY: ACM Press. Socci, A., D. (2007): Are TV stations undermining the standards and credibility of weather forecasting and meteorology? Bulletin of the American Meteorological Society, 88, no. 4, 578-580. Stark A., P., Quinn, S., Edmonds, R. (2007): Eyetracking the news: a study of print and online reading. St. Petersburg, FL: The Poynter Institute for Media Studies. Stewart, Al., E. (2006): Assessing the Human Experience of Weather and Climate: A Further Explanation of Weather Salience. Preprints, AMS Forum: Environmental Risk and Impacts on Society: Successes and Challenges; Atlanta, GA; Amer. Meteor. Soc., CD-ROM, 1.6. Tobii Eye Tracking (2010): An Introduction to Eye Tracking and Tobii Eye Trackers. Tobii Technology AB. Tobii T60 and T120 Eye Tracker User Manual, Revision 4 (2011): Tobii Technology AB. Trobec, J. (2007): Emerging television weather presenting strategies in the United States. EMS7/ECAM8 Abstracts, Vol. 4, EMS2007-A-00680, 2007 7th EMS Annual Meeting / 8th ECAM. Waterworth, E., L., Haggkvist, M., Jalkanen, K., Olsson, S., Waterworth, J., Wimelius, H. (2003): The Exploratorium: An Environment to Explore your Feelings. Psychology Journal, Volume 1, Number 3, pg. 189-201. White, S., J. (2008): Eye movement control during reading: Effects of word frequency and orthographic familiarity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance.   71   World Meteorological Organization (2005): Guidelines on weather broadcasting and the use of radio for the delivery of weather information. PWS 12, WMO/TD No. 1278. Yaros, R., A., Cook, A., E. (2011): Attention Versus Learning of Online Content: Preliminary Findings from an Eye-Tracking Study. International Journal of Cyber Behavior, Psychology and Learning (IJCBPL). Volume 1, Issue 4.   72   Chapter 4 Severe Weather Warning Communication: Factors impacting audience attention and retention of information during warnings. A wide variety of natural hazards impact the U.S. inflicting personal and economic tolls on the public. Communicating warnings about impending natural hazards is an important duty of weathercasters working for news organizations. On television, hazard warnings are typically conveyed through live radar, storm chasers, live newscasts, and warning scrolls. The effectiveness of this traditional approach, however, is unclear, especially given the lack of attention some members of the general public pay to these warnings. A study comparing individual responses to a traditional warning, an animated warning, and an audio warning was undertaken to evaluate the impact of delivery methods, on viewer retention, and viewer preferences during severe weather warnings. A Tobii T60 eye tracker was used to document visual interactions with onscreen warnings. Results indicate that viewers of the animated warning retained more pertinent information about the tornado warning than viewers of the traditional warning, and retention during the traditional warning was equivalent to that of the audio warning. In addition, gaze patterns for the traditional warning were much more diffuse than for the animated warning, suggesting that attention was more focused on the animation than the live video. Retention and preference data suggest that while traditional warnings do provide sufficient information for people to act upon, inclusion of novel animated elements may reinforce retention of the warning information. In addition, modifications to reduce visual complexity of traditional warnings may positively impact viewer attention to individual warning elements. Future studies will consider the   73   effectiveness of a hybrid warning containing both traditional and animated components. Development and analysis of a hybrid warning will allow for identification of warning elements that positively impact viewer attention and retention of warning information presented during traditional televised broadcasts. This information may be used to advance current severe weather warning communication techniques and increase public awareness during severe weather events. 4.1 Introduction A wide range of factors influences the effectiveness of severe weather warnings. Traditional elements, such as live weathercaster reporting, radar imagery and warning scrolls appearing at the bottom of the television screen are typical of live warning broadcasts (WMO 2005). Recent developments designed to enhance warning effectiveness include experimental impact-based warnings that include text containing enhanced “storm impact” information (CRH/NOAA 2013) and personalized texts to cell phones (NWS 2010). Despite a long history of traditional television warnings and new advances made to severe weather warning delivery and communication, significant impact to property and lives continues to be a major consequence associated with severe weather throughout the United States (Folger 2013). Improvements to current warning mechanisms may lead to additional avoidance of these consequences, and result in a reduction of the lives and property lost during severe weather events. The communication and delivery of information during a severe weather event share similar obstacles with a variety of natural hazards. Natural hazards exist in many forms throughout the United States (USGS 2007). In fact, the U.S. is rated among the top five countries over the last decade to be hit most   74   frequently with natural disaster events (Sapir, et. al. 2012). This places the population at risk from dangers associated with volcanoes, earthquakes, landslides, drought, fire, and severe weather. Severe weather, in the form of blizzards, hurricanes, and thunderstorms, pose the most frequently experienced risk (Smith 2013). Of these, thunderstorms occur most frequently throughout the United States (NOAA 2010). Thunderstorms produce lightning, tornadoes, straight-line winds, floods, and hail; together, these average about three billion dollars in insured losses and over 65 fatalities each year (NOAA 2010). In recent years, these losses have grown considerably and tornadoes have taken a lead role in damage associated with severe thunderstorms (Munich Re 2012). Recent events, such as the 2011 outbreak in Joplin Missouri, which claimed 161 lives and the 2013 event in Moore Oklahoma that killed 24 (Kuligowski et. al. 2013), represent a recent increase in severe outbreaks which have resulted in record property losses and fatalities associated with tornadoes (NCDC 2013). The destructive properties of tornadoes are the leading cause of property losses and the second leading cause of thunderstorm related fatalities (Folger 2013). In fact, during the 2011 extreme tornado outbreak (of which Joplin was a part), Munich Re and the National Climatic Data Center reported over 20 billion dollars in damage and 552 lives lost due to tornadoes alone (Simmons, et. al. 2011). The ability to reduce the impact of severe thunderstorms and tornadoes typically rests in efforts that focus on storm detection and warning systems developed by the National Oceanic and Atmospheric Administration (NOAA) and the National Weather Service (NWS), in conjunction with institutions focusing on severe weather detection and prediction (Folger   75   2013 & NOAA 2013). Recent research, however, cites policy, demographic and cognitive factors as potential elements that can increase fatalities associated with tornado fatalities (Folger 2013). Policy related factors, such as land use and rezoning practices, can influence the number of tornado fatalities by exposing the population to areas of greater risk of tornado activity (Brenner 1995). This may occur when land is developed in tornado prone areas, or existing structures are not required to have tornado safe shelters included in their design. Since people are more likely to seek shelter during tornado warnings, compared to other severe weather events, the type of shelter available may also affect a community’s safety (Folger 2013). Demographic factors, such as income, may also influence exposure to tornado risk. Manufactured homes, for example, are more prone to damage by tornadoes than traditional construction. Since a large number of low- and moderate-income families live in manufactured housing in tornado prone areas, tornado risk is generally increased in these situations, unless alternate tornadosafe structures are provided by the community (Sutter 2010). Cognitive factors, such as experience and knowledge, impact decisions made by the public regarding tornado warnings and may influence response to warnings. Research has shown that past tornado warning experience may negatively impact an individual’s response to tornado warnings when compared to those who have never experienced a tornado warning (Drost 2013). For instance, if previous instances exist in which an individual experienced a tornado warning with no subsequent tornado event they may be more likely to dismiss the warning. In some cases they may also look for additional confirming evidence of a tornadoes existence before seeking shelter (Mileti   76   and Sorensen 1990). Interestingly, severe weather knowledge positively influences individual and community response to severe weather warnings (Beer and Hamilton 2002 & Drost 2013). Increased knowledge among individuals and communities about their potential exposure to tornado risk increases awareness of the danger, and this heightened awareness promotes more prudent assessment of the potential risk and better decision-making (Beer and Hamilton 2002). Although severe weather experience is unique to individuals, knowledge of severe weather is available in many forms; traditional education, informal exposure through day-to-day activities, and media coverage of severe events. Knowledge, therefore, can be accessed through TV weather forecasts, social media, and Internet sources (Lazo 2009). In turn, these sources rely heavily on National Oceanic and Atmospheric Administration (NOAA) data that is supplied to the National Weather Service (NWS 2013), providing a direct link between scientists and the public. A strategic element of the NOAA mission focuses on understanding and predicting changes in weather (NOAA 2013). Much of this mission is accomplished through delivering daily weather information and storm warnings in conjunction with the National Weather Service (NWS). The NWS supplies a wide range of data and related weather products that are used by both the government and private sector (NWS 2013). Media outlets utilize these data to communicate weather information to the public. Technologies that deliver this information to the public range from personal devices (cell phones, PDA’s) to traditional newspapers and TV broadcasts (Drost 2013 and Lazo 2009). When utilizing televised broadcasts for severe weather information, the public may choose from local TV stations or national enterprises such as The Weather   77   Channel. Regardless of the choice of information, in extreme weather events a concise and timely forecast is crucial for decision-making to reduce losses to property and of life (Mileti and Sorensen 1990). Televised severe weather warning broadcasts utilize a number of visual elements to communicate information. Television producers assume that viewers have the ability to attend to both visual elements and verbal information being presented by the weathercaster simultaneously (Bergen, Grimes and Potter 2005). While accessing verbal and visual information simultaneously is quite feasible (Baddeley and Hitch 1974), attending to multiple visual elements at the same time represents a challenging visual task (VanRullen, et. al. 2007). As warning broadcasts become more visually complex through the addition of multiple screen images or elements (radar, live footage, warning scrolls, etc.), determining if the additional content provides added benefit to communicating warning information, or is instead a distracter to effective communication, becomes essential (Josephson and Holmes 2006). It is possible in some instances that less information, or fewer methods of conveying information may be more effective. Reduction of screen clutter represents a potential for improved viewer attention since the inclusion of multiple visual elements has been shown to negatively impact the effectiveness of the main broadcast message (IPG 2011) and reduces viewer comprehension of the information being presented (Bergen, Grimes and Potter 2005). Typically, traditional broadcasts include radar imagery, storm chasers, warning scrolls appearing at the bottom of the screen, and live weathercaster coverage of the event (WMO 2005). While these approaches attempt to incorporate multiple modes of   78   communication, their effectiveness and utilization by the public has not been entirely successful, as witnessed by the public’s frequent lack of response to severe weather warnings (NOAA 2008 and Lazo 2009). In order to increase the effectiveness of current warnings, a better understanding of public preferences contained within traditional broadcasts is necessary. This study attempts to investigate the effectiveness of warning elements and delivery methods associated with traditional televised severe weather warning broadcasts. It endeavors to understand the impacts of individual warning elements on public knowledge and retention and determine communication preferences during severe weather warnings. In this work, we compare delivery methods, retained knowledge, and preferences across three warning types: a traditional televised broadcast warning, an animated warning, and an audio warning. By understanding the efficacy of severe weather warning delivery methods, severe warning elements, and public retention of the information contained within, warnings may be produced that will reduce loss of lives and property during severe events. In this study, we examined whether differences in the conveyance (traditional, animated, audio) of severe weather warnings and message elements (radar, scrolls, live coverage) each contain impact viewer retention of the information contained in the warning. Participant preferences for warning information were also investigated to determine what content is considered most useful when facing decisions during severe weather warnings. This information was collected utilizing a multiple data collection mechanisms. These include gaze data collected during eye tracking, participant’s response to retention and preference surveys, and participant opinions expressed during think aloud questions. Gaze data   79   were converted to bee swarm outputs in order to ascertain where participant’s attention was focused during the warning clips. Recording participants eye movements using eye tracking equipment and software allows for the discovery of where individuals are looking and for how long, during the severe weather warnings. These fixations occur when a participant spends a defined length of time on a particular detail contained within the severe weather warning (Tobii 2010). A participant’s fixation and length of time spent on a particular detail is often indicative of the attention that is being paid to that area or item being viewed (Waterworth, et. al. 2003). Data from the retention and preference questionnaires were used to determine the amount of warning information participant’s retained after viewing the severe weather warning, and to identify warning elements they found most useful. Think aloud responses allowed participants to express their opinions regarding the warning model they had viewed. Results of the study can inform best practices for constructing a severe weather warning that positively impacts participant retention. 4.2 Methods This study occurred during an hour-long eye tracking experiment that consisted of four separate and unique studies. Participants were recruited through listserv and email solicitation at a large midwestern institution. Ninety members of the general public, undergraduate and graduate students, faculty, and staff, were paid $20 each for their participation. The population consisted of 59% female and 41% male participants. Ages of the participants ranged from 18 to 46 years, with a median of 23 years. Seventy-six of the participants were Caucasian, 18% Asian, 4% Black and 2% were self-coded as unspecified. Seventy-four percent of the participants were non-science   80   majors. As a whole the study participants had reported completion of a total of 42 Earth science related courses. Each participant was exposed to one of three unique severe weather warning treatments, consisting of two video clips (Figure 4.1) and one audio clip, through random assignment. All the participants also completed five questionnaires. The audio and video clips were of similar length (84-95 seconds) and contained nearly identical audio. Any differences in audio were related to slight differences in the references to warning visuals. The traditional video clip (TRADITIONAL) was a portion of an actual severe weather warning broadcast live as an interruption to a normal television broadcast. The animated video clip (ANIMATED) contained the same warning information as the TRADTIONAL clip and varied on slightly in length due to the addition of precautionary recommendations. The Animated clip was completely animated and contained no “live” components and contained the same audio as the AUDIO clip.. The audio clip (AUDIO) contained the same audio as the ANIMATED model without an accompanying visual component. The severe weather warning clips were viewed on a Tobii T60 eye tracker that allowed capture of eye movements during video clip viewing. Figure 4.1: Example of Traditional and Animated media clips. The Audio only clip contains no images and is not illustrated here.   81   4.2.1 Data Collection Data were collected in three successive steps during this study; pre-eye tracking, during eye tracking, and post eye tracking (Table 4.1). First, participants were asked to complete a weather knowledge and weather experience questionnaire. Next, participants were given a brief explanation of the Tobii T60 eye tracker before proceeding to the actual experiment. Participants were then exposed to one of the three severe weather media treatments. After completing the eye tracking portion of the study, participants were asked to complete three additional questionnaires; a severe weather retention questionnaire containing questions directly related to the weather warning, a weather model preference questionnaire that ranked model elements (radar, live footage, warning scrolls, etc.) and a questionnaire probing Earth science course experience and participant demographics. At the conclusion of the study, participants were asked several think-aloud questions to gain insight into their preferences for severe weather communication and opinions on the warning clip they previously viewed. Table 4.1: Data source descriptions and timing during the study. Data Sources Weather knowledge questionnaire Measurement Participant knowledge of severe weather and tornado facts and precautionary measures to take during warnings Timing Pre-eye tracking Two questions recorded participants’ thoughts regarding severe weather terminology. Weather experience questionnaire Four questions inventoried participants exposure to severe weather, specifically tornadoes Six questions assessed their attitudes and emotions towards severe weather and community response to warnings     82   Pre-eye tracking Table 4.1 (cont’d) Video eye tracking Captured fixation and gaze information to determine patterns of attention during video clips During eye tracking Retention questionnaire Participant retention of information communicated during the video clip Post eye tracking Preference questionnaire Four rank order questions determined participant preferences for warning communication and what is most helpful in making related decisions. Two Likert questions assessed the participant’s difficulty in understanding warnings. One Openended question addressed warning elements usefulness. Post eye tracking Domain experience questionnaire Recorded geoscience experience and demographic information Post eye tracking Think aloud questions Captured participants’ reaction and comments about the media clip they experienced Post eye tracking 4.2.2 Data Sources Data were collected through questionnaires, eye tracking, and think-aloud questions: 1) The Weather Knowledge Questionnaire (KNOWLEDGEQ) consisted of eleven questions pertaining to tornado development, severe weather facts, and recommended precautionary measures. The KNOWLEDGEQ was validated in Drost (2013) and measures weather knowledge among study participants to determine their understanding of common weather terms used during severe events. 2) The Weather Experience Questionnaire (EXPERIENCEQ) consisted of twelve questions divided into three categories: Two questions asked for participant reaction to the terms “severe weather warning” and “tornado”, four inventoried the participants exposure to severe weather, specifically tornadoes, and the remaining six evaluated attitudes and emotions towards severe weather and community response to warnings.   83   The EXPERIENCEQ questionnaire validation was previously established in Drost (2013). 3) Exposure to one of three severe weather warning models: Traditional media clip (TRADITIONAL), Animated video clip (ANIMATED), and an Audio media clip (AUDIO). . These models were displayed on a Tobii T60 Eye Tracker, which was used to capture participant fixations and gaze information. These eye tracking data were used to discern patterns of viewing that suggest attention or distraction during viewing. This information was then compared to retention data in order to gauge the impact of multiple screen elements on participant attention and retention and the use of novel animated elements to increase attention and retention of warning information. 4) The Severe Weather Retention Questionnaire (RETENTIONQ) was made up of ten multiple choice questions specific to the severe weather warning clip participants were exposed to during the eye-tracking session. The questions were used to determine what specific information participants remembered while viewing/listening to their particular warning model. 5) The Severe Weather Model Preference Survey (PREFERENCEQ) contained four rank order questions specific to the severe weather warning model participants were exposed to during the experiment. The PREFERENCEQ was completed after the experiment and questions focused on the participants’ preferences for elements used in the warning they had viewed or heard (radar imagery, location determination, weathercaster delivery, etc.). In addition, two questions assessing participants’ difficulty in understanding severe weather warnings and an open-ended question asking about most useful elements of warnings were included. The survey was intended to compare   84   alternative warning elements to those currently in place, and to determine what participants consider most useful, or preferred, during severe weather warnings. 6) A demographic survey collected information on participants educational major, gender, age, ethnicity and family educational background, as well as exposure to Earth science courses. 7) Participants were asked think aloud questions at the conclusion of the experiment. These open ended questions allowed participants to express their views and opinions about the severe weather warning clip they experienced and preferences for severe weather communication, if any. 4.2.3 Data Analysis Three types of data were analyzed: eye tracking data, survey data, and thinkaloud data (justification of methods are found in Appendix 4H). Eye tracking gaze data from the TRADITIONAL and ANIMATED treatments were used to generate participants gaze points simultaneously over the course of the video clip. This data allows for the comparison of the gaze patterns of multiple participants for each video warning clip. The resulting video output produced is known as a bee swarm. These bee swarm videos were analyzed to determine the extent of attention or distraction experienced by participants during the experiment. Descriptive statistics were used to explain the results of the KNOWLEDGEQ, EXPERIENCEQ, RETENTIONQ, PREFERENCEQ and DEQ data in order to provide summaries of the data collected during the experiment. A statistical analysis of the data provided by the KNOWLEDGEQ, EXPERIENCEQ was performed to determine similarity among the participants and the potential impact on retention. Additional statistical analysis was performed on the RETENTIONQ,   85   PREFERENCEQ data to ascertain the whether a difference existed among the participants and the warning model they experienced. The KNOWLEDGEQ evaluated the participant’s level of familiarity with severe weather facts, tornado development and recommended precautionary measures. Data from the KNOWLEDGEQ were used as one of the three pre-measures (knowledge, experience, trust) performed before the eye tracking session began. The EXPERIENCEQ was meant to assess the participant’s experience with warning messages, severe weather, and tornadoes by inventorying actual experience with these items and assigning a score based on the participant’s responses. Additional Likert questions contained within the EXPERIENCEQ measured participant emotions about tornado warnings and tornadoes during the pre-stimuli data collection. Six questions regarding severe weather warnings were rated on a Likert scale where 1 indicated, “strongly agree” and 5 indicated, “strongly disagree”. A factor analysis was performed on the six Likert questions contained within the EXPERIENCEQ to determine if a correlation existed that would lead to a common scale of participant trust in weather warnings. The results of the knowledge, experience, and trust data were then analyzed to determine if they were similar for all models. An independent samples T-test was performed on the knowledge, experience and trust data to determine if there were any statistical differences across the three groups. Significant statistical differences may indicate that knowledge, experience and trust may have the ability to influence participant performance on other instruments contained within the study.   86   Next, an independent samples T-test was performed comparing the RETENTIONQ data among all three warning models to determine if significant differences existed between the participant questionnaire results. The analysis was based on questions 1 through 7 of the RETENTIONQ data since the existing model did not contain specific references to the remaining questions (8 through 10). These questions queried participants about specific precautionary measures associated with tornado warnings. As a final measure of the potential impacts of knowledge, experience and trust on the three groups performance on the RETENTIONQ, a linear regression was run to determine if there were any main or interaction effects. The severe weather model preference questionnaire (PREFERENCEQ) was used to determine elements of a severe weather warning broadcast that participants considered most helpful when attempting to understand and make decisions. The PREFERENCEQ contained similar questions for each weather warning model, but was altered when necessary due to inherent differences in the three treatments. In these instances data were recorded as n/a. Participants indicated their preferences by determining a rank order for each model element. The PREFERENCEQ also contained two questions that assessed the level of difficulty in understanding the warning model they viewed and how it compared to a traditional TV broadcast. They rated their models on a Likert scale where 1 was “not difficult” and 5 was “very difficult”. A factor analysis was performed on the two Likert questions contained within the PREFERENCEQ. An independent samples T-test was performed on “difficulty” questions contained in the EXPERIENCEQ for all three models   87   to determine if differences existed between the groups. In addition, a linear regression was performed to ascertain what impacts difficulty may have on the RETENTIONQ scores. Individual screen shots taken from the Tobii T60 eye tracker bee swarm data for both the TRADITIONAL and ANIMATED clips were initially identified based on 5-second increments during both warning videos. These screen shots were then compared and paired according to the similarity of the content and audio elements. These example shots displayed station and weathercaster identification, warning level, current storm track and location and predicted future storm movements. These elements were all representative of questions asked on the RETENTIONQ and PREFERENCEQ. Participant gaze was represented in the bee swarm data by the black dots on the images and allowed us to quantitatively analyze participants gaze during individual screen shots. In order to compare participant gaze for each video warning video model, individual scenes were captured every five seconds to quantify the subject’s gaze, resulting in 18 individual shots. Next, screen shots containing identical images or information exclusive to the individual media scene were eliminated and as a result 15 scene shots were retained. A rubric was developed that allowed researchers to code the viewer’s gaze as either concentrated or diffuse. Two authors’ coded 15 scene shots for each warning clip. Each set of clips was coded separately, and a reliability check yielded an independent rating of 84% agreement. Any inconsistencies discovered during the process were discussed between the two authors and worked through until a mutual agreement was reached.   88   Lastly, participants were asked think aloud questions about the severe weather warning clip they viewed and preferences for communication during warnings. 4.3 Results 4.3.1 Participant Results – Knowledge, Experience and Trust The KNOWLEDGEQ and EXPERIENCEQ provided useful information for determining the level of severe weather information and past experience for the study population. In addition, trust questions contained within the EXPEREINCEQ provided insight into the population’s level of confidence in warning information and recommendations for taking precautionary measures. The difficulty questions contained within the PREFERENCEQ also provided information on the ease of understanding traditional severe weather information. The population exhibited moderate levels of understanding of weather. The weather knowledge questionnaire (KNOWLEDGEQ) tested understanding of conditions necessary for tornado development, frequency, timing and weather statements associated with approaching storms, as well as, common misconceptions. Participants had a median score of 60% on the KNOWLEDGEQ. Several questions stood out in participant responses. When asked the typical speed of a tornado and how much advanced warning is given only 20% of the respondents answered correctly. Somewhat more disturbing is the fact that only 50% of the respondents understood that a “warning” is the most urgent National Weather Service Statement. The weather experience questionnaire (EXPERIENCEQ) consisted of eleven questions and inventoried participant’s weather experiences and thoughts/emotions concerning severe weather. The majority of participants reported experiencing some   89   form of severe weather: results indicated that 86% of the participants experienced a severe weather warning at least once a year while 50% had experienced an actual severe weather event. Of that 50%, within the last two years, 40% experienced a severe thunderstorm, 36% experienced heavy or blowing snow, 23% experienced torrential rain or flooding, 22% experienced damaging winds, and 3% experienced a tornado. Additionally, 13% of the participants reported physically experiencing a tornado over two years ago ranging in strength from an EF0 to EF4 (from the Enhanced Fujita Scale) based on their judgment that were provided as part of the questionnaire. In addition to knowledge and experience, the level of trust exhibited by participants was measured using six Likert scale questions contained within the PREFERENCEQ (Table 4.2). A factor analysis was performed and retained the first four of the six questions, yielding a "Trust in Weather Warnings" (Trust) scale calculated from the average of these four items. For our sample, the scale yielded a Cronbach's alpha of 0.69, acceptable for use as an aggregate sample-level measure. The four-item trust scale resulted in an overall average of 2.44 indicating that the participant level of trust in weather warnings fell between agree and neutral. Table 4.2: Likert scale questions and participant’s (n=90) responses. 1 = strongly agree, 5 = strongly disagree. Question 1. I trust the information contained in severe weather warnings. 2. Tornado warnings are indicative of a serious threat to my life and property. 3. A severe weather warning evokes negative emotions for me. 4. Existing community tornado preparedness plans keep me safe. 5. Instinct guides my decisions when faced with severe weather warnings. 6. The image of a tornado evokes positive emotions for me. Participant’s Response 1.97 2.27 2.71 2.83 2.67 4.27 Data collected prior to the eye tracking portion of the study indicated all groups possessed a moderate amount of knowledge, mean score of 6.5 ± 1.5 (maximum of   90   11), and experience, mean score of 1.49 ± .81 (maximum of 3). A trust mean score of 2.44 ± .72 (maximum of 5) indicated that the groups neither trusted nor distrusted severe weather warnings. Subsequent independent samples T-tests indicated that no significant differences existed among the three groups for knowledge, experience and trust (Appendix G). Differences in these data could have potentially impacted variations in RETENTIONQ data for the three severe weather warning models. 4.3.2 Model Results – Retention and Preference The RETENTIONQ determined the amount of information the study population retained immediately after viewing the severe weather warning clip and provides insight into the effectiveness of the warning model once other factors that may influence participant retention are eliminated. The PREFERENCEQ provided information relevant to participant preferences for severe weather warning delivery methods and content contained within the actual warning. Results of the RETENTIONQ varied among the three warning models. Overall, the participant’s average score for all three models was 38%. Individually, participants viewing the ANIMATED model scored 46%; those viewing the TRADITIONAL model scored 35%, while those viewing the AUDIO model scored 36% on the RETENTIONQ. Independent-samples t-tests were conducted to compare RETENTIONQ scores for the ANIMATED group, TRADITIONAL group, and AUDIO group. There was a significant difference in the scores for the ANIMATED (M=4.66, SD=1.10) and the TRADITIONAL (M=3.5, SD=1.66) groupings; t(58)=-2.94, p=.005. There was also a significant difference in the scores for the ANIMATED (M=4.66, SD=1.10) and the AUDIO (M=3.6, SD 1.48) groupings; t(58)=2.87, p=.006). These results suggest that the   91   animated severe weather warning had a greater effect on participant retention of information than the traditional severe weather warning and the audio warning. In order to determine if knowledge, experience and/or trust impacted RETENTIONQ results, a linear regression was performed. We found that knowledge (Beta=-.052, p>.33), experience (Beta=.178, p>.11) and trust (Beta=-.067, p>.58) were not significant predictors for retention, indicating that knowledge, experience and trust had no main or interaction effects within the groups. RETENTIONQ scores for participants were directly related to the individual model viewed during the study. The ANIMATED groups performance was statistically significant to the performance of TRADITIONAL and AUDIO participants. The PREFERENCEQ asked participants to list in rank order the most helpful resources, aspects, and information when attempting to understand information contained in the warning model they viewed. Resources included a range of four to seven elements, such as radar imagery and weathercaster in-studio report, depending on the warning model viewed (Appendix D, E, F). In this instance participants who viewed the TRADITIONAL and ANIMATED model indicated that the radar image was the most helpful resource. Since the AUDIO participant viewed no video, the option to choose that response was not included in their questionnaire. These participants selected the weathercaster’s live in-studio report of storm location and movement as the most helpful resource. Next, depending on the warning model, participant’s ranked three to six elements as the most helpful aspect in determining where the storm is located or moving in relation to their location. Examples of these elements are towns and cities, highways   92   and roads and county boundary lines and are contained in appendix 4D, 4E and 4F. The TRADITIONAL and ANIMATED group ranked names of towns and cities first, while the AUDIO group ranked the weathercaster’s description of storm location and movement first and highways and roads second. This difference may be due to the lack of visual content associated with the audio warning or unfamiliarity with one’s geographical surrounding. Participants were then asked to imagine that they were watching television and the program was interrupted by a message from the National Weather Service (NWS) indicating a tornado was predicted for their area. They were then asked to select what information would be helpful in making a decision on how to respond to the severe weather warning. Again, these choices are representative of those found in traditional televised severe weather warnings and examples may be found in appendix 4D, 4E and 4F. In this instance, all three groups (TRADITIONAL, ANIMATED, AUDIO) selected the radar image, NWS warning message and the forecaster’s recommendation as the top three informational sources. Considering the same scenario as the previous question, where they were asked to imagine that they were watching television and the program was interrupted by a message from the NWS indicating a tornado was predicted for their area, participants were asked to list in rank order the most helpful information in making a decision when the NWS predicts a tornado for their area. All groups (TRADITIONAL, ANIMATED, AUDIO) chose radar image as the most helpful. Two Likert scale questions were used in the PREFERENCEQ to determine the participant’s difficulty understanding the severe weather warning model they viewed, and how they would rate the ease of understanding compared to a traditional TV   93   broadcast. Responses for question 1 indicated a comparable level of difficulty among all three groups (TRADITIONAL, ANIMATED, AUDIO) and the video clip they viewed. Question 2 indicated that the ANIMATED and AUDIO clip are harder to understand than a traditional TV broadcast, but the TRADITIONAL warning clip ranked on par to the traditional TV broadcast (Table 4.3). Table 4.3: Participant’s Likert scale responses to the difficulty and ease of understanding their severe weather warning model. 1=not difficult, 5=very difficulty. Question Model Participant’s response TRADITIONAL 1.93 1) How difficult did you find the severe weather ANIMATED 2.00 warning to understand? AUDIO 2.33 TRADITIONAL 1.73 2) Compared to live media (TV) broadcasts of severe ANIMATED 2.40 weather warnings rate your ease of understanding of the recorded warning. AUDIO 2.97 An independent samples T-test was performed on the data to determine if the data were significantly different from each other. Results for question 1 indicate a significant difference in the scores for the AUDIO (M=2.33, SD=.80) and the TRADITIONAL (M=1.93, SD=.74) groupings; t(58)=-2.00, p=0.049 but not for the AUDIO compared to the ANIMATED (M=2.00, SD=.91) group: t(58)=-1.51, p=.138. These results suggest that the AUDIO group found their severe weather warning clip somewhat more difficult to understand than the TRADITIONAL and ANIMATED groups. Results for question 2 indicate there was a significant difference in the scores for the ANIMATED (M=2.4, SD=1.13) and the TRADITIONAL (M=1.73, SD=.74) groupings; t(58)=-2.70, p=.009 and the AUDIO (M=2.97, SD=1.03) and TRADITIONAL(M=1.73, SD=.74) groupings; t(58)=-5.31, p<=.001 These results suggest that the ANIMATED and AUDIO groups found the severe weather warning clip they viewed more difficult to understand than a traditional TV severe weather warning broadcast. Despite the fact   94   that the ANIMATED group found the warning clip more difficult to understand than the TRADITIONAL group, they outperformed the TRADITIONAL group on the RETENTIONQ. It is possible that the difficulty in understanding forced more attention on the ANIMATED video clip by the viewers (as evidenced by bee swarm data) and resulted in increased retention. A final question contained in the PREFERENCEQ solicited participants opinions on what element of the severe weather warning model they viewed would be most useful when making a decision on what actions to take in the event of a tornado warning. Those who viewed the TRADITIONAL model most often-mentioned radar imagery, storm direction information and the timing and location of the event as most useful. Participants who viewed the ANIMATED model also mentioned radar imagery and storm direction information, but differed by then mentioning precautionary measures as most useful. The AUDIO group mentioned timing and location and precautionary measures as most useful and made no mention of radar imagery. In order to compare differences in participant gaze between the two warning clips, bee swarm data were generated indicating where participants were looking during the clips Figure 4.2). A comparison of the TRADITIONAL and ANIMATED bee swarm data indicated that for the TRADITIONAL warning clips participants exhibited a diffuse pattern of gaze during 33% of the selected clips. In comparison, participants viewing the ANIMATED warning clips exhibited a diffuse pattern in less than 1% of the selected clips. Several of the clips contained multiple elements (weathercaster, radar, warning scroll), which resulted in participant gaze being divided among the individual elements   95   being displayed. These were not considered to be diffuse as long as participant gaze was concentrated on the individual elements. Figure 4.2: Series of images showing participant gaze for both the TRADITIONAL (Trad) and ANIMATED (Anim) media clip. Individual participant’s gaze is indicated by the black dots. Clip Trad1 is representative of a “diffuse” gaze pattern; Trad2 is representative of a “concentrated” gaze pattern. Clip Anim1 is representative of a gaze “concentrated” on multiple elements; Anim2 is representative of a “concentrated” gaze pattern. At the conclusion of the study participants were asked to comment on the severe weather warning model they had previously viewed. This was meant to provide information not easily captured as part of a questionnaire and allow the three groups to express their opinions on the warning model they viewed. The TRADITIONAL group frequently expressed feelings of confusion and cited the abundance of information being provided during the warning as “a lot to consider”. The ANIMATED group overall found the animated warning enjoyable but mention it would be “hard to take seriously”. Lastly, the AUDIO group mentioned that the lack of visuals contributed to the difficulty in remembering warning information and that they needed to “concentrate on the message” to remember what was being communicated.   96   4.4 Discussion and Conclusions Effective severe weather warnings are essential to providing the population with the information needed to take action when threatening weather exists. Warnings that contain salient information and are delivered to the public on a timely basis provide ample opportunity for precautionary measures to be taken to protect lives and property. Traditional severe weather warnings contain multiple elements such as radar, live reports, warning scrolls, and weathercaster recommendations that convey storm information to the population. This relevant information allows the public to make decisions on what steps to take to ensure their safety. This study examined traditional severe weather warnings and the communication elements they contain to gain a perspective of how information retention is impacted and what elements are considered most useful during threatening conditions. Participants viewed a traditional severe weather warning, animated warning and an audio warning containing nearly identical severe weather warning information. Additional information pertaining to participant knowledge, experience and warning preferences was collected for each warning treatment. Overall, participant knowledge of basic severe weather concepts was moderate. Participants demonstrated a lack of knowledge and understanding of weather statements, tornado speed and lead-time available to take precautionary measures. Understanding the NWS weather statement information and knowing when to take precautionary measures is an important first step to ensuring personal safety during threatening weather. Knowledge of a tornado’s speed and direction figure prominently in   97   determining an individual’s course of action and heighten the need for immediate action. Combined, this lack of understanding makes for a potentially disastrous situation. While the majority of participants had experienced a severe weather warning only a small percentage had recent exposure to a tornado. As a whole, participant’s trust of the information contained in the warnings and potential for serious harm is somewhat neutral. The neutral attitude toward warning information and potential for threats to their lives and property exposes them to additional danger when combined with the lack of knowledge about severe weather location and movement. Perhaps the frequency in which participants had experienced warnings and subsequent lack of understanding the warning messages themselves contribute to the neutral attitude toward the perceived potential for harm. Our data suggest that the ANIMATED treatment was better than TRADITIONAL at communicating information in the weather warning. The animated severe weather warning was shown to statistically impact participant retention of severe weather information when compared to the group who viewed the traditional and audio warning. Not only did the ANIMATED group retain more of the severe weather warning information contained in the clip, but bee swarm data also support the evidence of increased attention on visually salient warning elements contained within the ANIMATED media clip. Participants viewing the ANIMATED model focused on specific emerging aspects of the warning in a highly concentrated fashion while participants viewing the TRADITIONAL model exhibited a somewhat more diffuse focus. Even though the ANIMATED group retained more warning information than the TRADITIONAL and AUDIO groups, they indicated that the ANIMATED warning clip was   98   more difficult to understand when compared to a traditional TV severe weather broadcast. The bee swarm data may also provide insight into this phenomenon. It is possible that the novel elements represented by the ANIMATED warning caused a more focused gaze on the particular warning elements which lead to the participants increasing the attention required to understand the information being provided by the media clip. This increased attention may be representative of the difficulty reported by the participants comparing the ANIMATED warning to a traditional warning broadcast. The increased attention on warning elements may also be indicative of the increase in information retention by the ANIMATED group. The inclusion of novel elements into severe weather warnings provides opportunities to further investigate the benefits and risks associated with supplementing traditional warnings with animated elements. Novel elements may initially provide a positive impact in viewer attention or retention of warning information. As a new source of communication, viewer’s attention may be drawn to the novel element, resulting in more effective delivery of weather warnings. However, once the novel element has become commonplace in warning messages it’s effectiveness may diminish as the “newness” fades. Preferences in severe weather warning elements were relatively consistent among all groups. Participants selected radar imagery, weathercaster information and NWS warning scroll as sources of storm intensity, location and future track as preferred sources of warning information. Although the ANIMATED and AUDIO groups warning models differed slightly with the inclusion of weathercaster recommendations for precautionary measures, all groups consistently rated this element among the most   99   helpful for making decisions during a severe weather event. The TRADITIONAL group would select the “recap of weather information” in place of “weathercaster recommendations” since recommendations were not included in the PREFERENCEQ for the traditional media clip. This indicates that updated weather information was of primary consideration to all groups. Additional elements rated useful among all groups were information associated with names of towns and cities, highways and roads, and county boundary lines. Although this information was ranked highly by all groups, the AUDIO group differed by selecting the “weathercaster description of location and movement” first. This may be explained by the context of where the viewer would be listening to an audio warning, such as an automobile, and/or to the lack of visual imagery associated with the depiction of these elements. Participant’s preference of severe weather warning elements contained within warning broadcasts suggests that warnings may contain information unhelpful to the viewer, and that information may lead to viewer distraction as evidenced by the scattered pattern of the bee swarm data in the TRADITIONAL warning clip. In addition, our study shows that traditional warning messages could be improved by reducing the number of components shown simultaneously that lead to viewer distraction. This combination, along with the fact that participant knowledge, experience and trust did not impact retention during the viewing of the three models, supports the inclusion of novel animated elements within traditional severe weather warnings. Care should be taken however, when including animated elements since participants often commented that a   100   warning that was completely animated would be taken less seriously than a traditional warning. These results support the development of a hybrid severe weather warning model that includes traditional and animated (or novel) elements. In order to accomplish the development of a hybrid warning we propose further studies that will incorporate data fielded from a larger population, including heads of households, in order to more aptly represent the general population. These studies will utilize gaze data to identify the most useful aspects of traditional and animated warning models and viewer attention information. This data will be combined with retention and preference data in order to create a hybrid severe weather warning model which will be compared to a traditional severe weather warning, and evaluated on its ability to positively impact viewer retention of warning information and attention to warning elements. It is likely positive impacts associated with elevated retention and attention will provide a sounder basis of viewer decision-making during severe weather events. These developments will build upon previous work in order to advance warning and communication techniques, leading to more effective public communication during severe weather events.   101   APPENDICES   102   Appendix 4A: Knowledge Questionnaire (KNOWLEDGEQ)   103   Appendix 4B: Experience Questionnaire (EXPERIENCEQ)   104   Appendix 4C: Retention Questionnaire (RETENTIONQ)   105   Appendix 4D: Traditional Model Questionnaire (PREFERENCEQ - TRAD)   106   Appendix 4D (cont’d)   107   Appendix 4E: Animated Model Questionnaire (PREFERENCEQ - ANIM)   108   Appendix 4E (cont’d)   109   Appendix 4F: Audio Model Questionnaire (PREFERENCEQ - AUDIO)   110   Appendix 4F (cont’d)   111   Appendix 4G: Independent T-Test Results Table 4G: Independent T-Test Results. Independent Ttest (Equal Model N Mean variances assumed) Knowledge 1 30 6.73 2 30 6.37 Experience 1 30 1.33 2 30 1.50 Trust 1 30 2.56 2 30 2.31 Knowledge 2 30 6.37 3 30 6.40 Experience 2 30 1.50 3 30 1.63 Trust 2 30 2.31 3 30 2.47 Knowledge 1 30 6.73 3 30 6.40 Experience 1 30 1.33 3 30 1.63 Trust 1 30 2.56 3 30 2.48   F Sig. t 1.53 1.30 .76 .82 .71 .83 1.30 2.02 .82 .85 .83 .62 1.53 2.02 .76 .85 .71 .62 .617 .436 1.001 58 .321 .298 .587 -.817 58 .417 .765 .385 1.211 58 .231 4.502 .038 -.076 58 .940 .009 .923 -.618 58 .539 3.099 .084 -.879 58 .383 1.932 .170 .719 58 .475 .157 .693 -.144 58 .155 .921 .341 .436 58 .664 112   df Sig. (2tailed) SD Appendix 4H: Analytical Methods Descriptive statistics were used to explain the results of the KNOWLEDGEQ, EXPERIENCEQ, RETENTIONQ, PREFERENCEQ and DEQ. Descriptive statistics are used to describe the base level data of a study and provide simple summaries. They are typically used as a base level of study information and provide a basis for further quantitative analysis of the study. For this study descriptive statistics were used to describe the level of knowledge and experience of the study population. It was also used to determine the amount of information retained after viewing the severe weather models and give insight into the study populations preferences for having severe weather information communication. Additional descriptive data also provided information on the gender and age of the study population. This information was used as a basis for determining additional statistical analysis of the data collected during the study. Further statistical analyses were performed on KNOWLEDGEQ, EXPERIENCEQ, RETENTIONQ, PREFERENCEQ and Trust data to determine differences among the participants and the model they experienced. Independent samples T-test’s were performed on knowledge, experience and trust to determine statistical differences across the three groups. In addition, an independent samples T-test was also performed comparing RETENTIONQ data among all three models to determine if differences existed between questionnaire results. Independent samples T-tests are used to compare the means of two independent samples, or populations. It is often utilized to test a hypothesis based on the difference between two different groups (samples). This study was composed of three different treatments (groups) and the Independent samples T-test’s were used to determine if significant differences existed among the groups viewing the three treatments in terms of their knowledge of severe weather, experience with severe weather and trust of the weather information being communicated during their specific treatment. This was completed in order to determine if these variables impacted results of participant retention of the information presented in their severe weather warning treatment. A T-test was also performed comparing participant retention scores to determine if significant differences existed in retention of the severe weather information communicated among the three groups. Finally, an independent samples T-test was performed on “difficulty” questions contained in the EXPERIENCEQ for all three models to determine if differences existed between the groups. A linear regression was run to determine if any main or interaction effects of knowledge, experience and trust were seen on the participant’s performance on the RETENTIONQ. Linear regressions are used to predict the impact of one or several variables on a second variable. For this study, the linear regression was used to determine if participant knowledge, experience or trust of severe weather had impact on the scores they received on the retention instrument. In addition, a linear regression was performed to ascertain what impacts difficulty may have on the RETENTIONQ scores.   113   A factor analysis was performed on the six Likert questions contained within the PREFERENCEQ used to determine the participant’s level of trust of weather warnings. A factor analysis may be used as a data reduction method. In these instances it attempts to generate a smaller amount of uncorrelated variables from a larger set of correlated variables. By this method it generates an output of variables that measure similar things from larger set. In this study a factor analysis was performed on the six Likert questions. Four of the six questions were retained during the analysis and the result was a scale that indicated the participant’s trust in weather warnings.   114   REFERENCES   115   REFERENCES Baddeley, A.D. and Hitch, G. (1974) Working memory. In The Psychology of Learning and Motivation (Bower, G.A., ed.), pp. 48–79, Academic Press. Beer, T., Hamilton, R., (2002): Natural Disaster Reduction, Safer Sustainable Communities: Making Better Decisions about Risk. ICSU Position Paper. IUGG Commission on Geophysical Risk & Sustainability and ICSU Committee on Disaster Reduction. Bergen, L., Grimes, T., Potter, D., (2005): How Attention Partitions itself during Simultaneous Message Presentations. Human Communication Research. Vol. 31 No. 3, July 2005 311-336. Brenner, S., Noji, E. K., (1995): Tornado Injuries as Related to Housing in the Plainfield Tornado. International Journal of Epidemiology. 23: 144. Central Region Headquarters (CRH/NOAA), (2013): www.crh.noaa.gov/images/crh/IBW Drost, R., (2013): Memory and decision-making: Determining action when the sirens sound. Weather, Climate and Society. January 2013. Folger, P., (2013): Severe Thunderstorms and Tornadoes in the United States. Congressional Research Service. 7-5700. www.crs.gov. R40097. IPG Media Lab, (2011): Are all screens created equal? A research study by the IPG Media Lab. Josephson, S., Holmes, M., (2006): Clutter or content? How on-screen enhancements affect how TV viewers scan and what they learn. Association for Computing Machinery, Inc. ETRA. Kuligowski, E. D., Phan, L. T., Levitan, M. L., Jorgensen, D. P., (2013): Preliminary Reconnaissance of the May 20, 2013, Newcastle-Moore Tornado in Oklahoma. National Institute of Standards and Technology. Lazo, J. K., Morss, R. E., and deMuth, J. L., (2009): 300 Billion Served: Sources, Perceptions, Uses, and Values of Weather Forecasts. BAMS, p. 785-798. Mileti, D. S., Sorenson, J. H., (1990): Communication of emergency public warnings. A social science perspective and State-of-the- Art Assessment. Oak Ridge National Laboratory, ORNL-6609. Munich Re, (2012): Severe weather in North America. Perils, Risks, Insurance.   116   National Climatic Data Center (NCDC) (2013): www.ncdc.noaa.gov/climateinformation/extreme-events. National Oceanic and Atmospheric Administration (NOAA) (2010): www.nws.noaa.gov/os/severeweather/resources. National Oceanic and Atmospheric Administration (NOAA) (2009): Service Assessment of the Super Tuesday Tornado Outbreak of February 5-6, 2008. National Oceanic and Atmospheric Administration (NOAA) (2013): www.noaa.gov/about-noaa. National Weather Service (NWS) (2010): Experimental Service Description Document (SDD). Experimental Mobile Decision Support Services (MDSS). Interactive NWS (iNWS): Warning Alert SMS Text and Email Messaging Services via Mobile Device Technologies. National Weather Service (NWS) (2013): www.nws.noaa.gov/mission. Sapir, D. G., Vos, F., Below, R., Ponserre, S., (2012): Annual Disaster Statistical Review: the numbers and trends. Centre for Research on the Epidemiology of Disasters (CRED) Institute of Health and Society (IRSS) Université catholique de Louvain – Brussels, Belgium. Simmons, K., Sutter, D., Pielke, R., (2012): Blown away: monetary and human impacts of the 2011 U.S. tornadoes. Extreme events and insurance: 2011 annus horribilis, p107-120. Smith, D. F., (2013): The Frequency of Severe Weather Events. EQECAT. ABS Group. Sutter, D., Simmons, K., M., (2010): Tornado Fatalities and Mobile Homes in the United States. Natural Hazards 53(1): 125-137. Tobii Eye Tracking (2010): An Introduction to Eye Tracking and Tobii Eye Trackers. Tobii Technology AB. United States Geological Survey (USGS) (2009): Natural Hazards – A National Threat. U.S. Department of the Interior, U.S. Geological Survey Fact sheet. VanRullen, R., Carlson, T., Cavanagh, P., (2007): The blinking spotlight of attention. PNAS 19204 - 19209, vol. 104, no. 49. Waterworth, E., L., Haggkvist, M., Jalkanen, K., Olsson, S., Waterworth, J., Wimelius, H. (2003): The Exploratorium: An Environment to Explore your Feelings. Psychology Journal, Volume 1, Number 3, pg. 189-201.   117   World Meteorological Organization (WMO) (2005): Guidelines on Weather Broadcasting and the use of Radio for the Delivery of Weather Information. WMO/TD No. 1278.   118