< a. $3,” . $3.. mm V I: . ¢ . .L ‘ , L5 . nh . V ,. 1.3.. , is a. 3,313 .. .. is. .a . aka; 3.2.... t} 15.... THFSIS 3. i003 , . LIBRARY Michigan State This is to certify mat the Umversuty thesis entitled COMPLETING A NOVEL TASK UNDER DIFFERENT SENSORY INPUT MODALITY CONDITIONS presented by Janelle Mane Rowe has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for the Master of degree in Audiology and Speech Arts Sciences Ma, Q, Sham/2mg MajéIr Professor's Signature 5/8/ 0 3 Date MSU is an Affirmative Action/Equal Opportunity Institution 44 .- A -—. -—-_ PLACE IN RETURN BOX to remove this checkout from your record. TO AVOID FINES return on or before date due. MAY BE RECALLED with earlier due date if requested. I DATE DUE DATE DUE DATE DUE LA? 2 9 2006 "GEM—9 ZUUE FEB 062007 12 3:331‘5} 6/01 c:/CIRC/DateDue.p65p15 COMPLETING A NOVEL TASK UNDER DIFFERENT SENSORY INPUT MODALITY CONDITIONS By Janelle Marie Rowe A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF ARTS Department of Audiology and Speech Sciences 2003 ABSTRACT COMPLETING A NOVEL TASK UNDER DIFFERENT SENSORY INPUT MODALITY CONDITIONS By Janelle Marie Rowe This study examined if verbal and non-verbal performances on a novel procedural task were differentially affected by (1.) the sensory modality of input and (2.) a time delay for displaying learning. One purpose was to determine if a modality bias existed for the group that received combined auditory, visual and tactual sensory inputs (AVT) versus the groups that received only auditory sensory input (A) or combined auditory and visual sensory inputs (AV). Another purpose was to see if certain modality conditions enhanced the retention of the new information. This study used 21 Caucasian, male, college students aged 18 to 21 years. It compared the accuracy of their non-verbal performances and of the procedural discourses provided by the individuals in each of the experimental groups. For the verbal performance, results showed a statistically significant difference between the AVT group and the other two groups (A and AV) after the 20-minute time delay but not before. With respect to the nonverbal performance, the A group performed significantly poorer than did the other two groups. These results have implications for future research and clinical work with traumatic brain injury patients (TBI). ACKNOWLEDGMENTS This thesis would not have been possible without the assistance, encouragement and support from many important people. First, I would like to thank Dr. Ida Stockman, my thesis chairperson, for providing me with guidance, direction and encouragement. I would also like to thank my thesis committee members, Dr. Jeffrey Marler and Dr. Mangala Sadasivan for providing me with the inspiration to pursue this thesis project. I would like to acknowledge and thank Ann Bruegger, Allison Pavlat, and Alicia Wetherall who willingly devoted their time to help analyze the collected data. In addition, I would like to express my gratitude to my husband-to-be, Jeffrey Neal for all of the many roles that he played during the course of this project. From my cameraman to my computer expert to my personal cheerleader, he was always there to support, encourage and assist me in any way possible. Last, but certainly NOT least, I would like to thank my family for their ever-constant love, encouragement and faith. Without your support this thesis would not have been possible. I love you all so much! TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES ................................................................................. vii CHAPTER 1 . INTRODUCTION TO PROBLEM FOR STUDY ............. 1 Characteristics of TBI ............................................................................. 1 Incidence of TBI ........................................................................... 1 Differential Diagnosis: Aphasia versus TBI .......................................... 2 Symptoms of TBI .......................................................................... 3 Intervention Perspectives ....................................... ' ................................. 8 Neuropsychological Approach ......................................................... 8 Functional Approach ..................................................................... 9 Rationale for the Present Study ............................................................... 11 Event Knowledge as the Root of Intelligent Behavior .......................... 11 Affolter and Bischofberger. The Model of a Root and Learning ............. 14 Theoretical Assumptions of Affolter and Bischofberger’s Model ............ 15 Statement of the Problem ...................................................................... 16 Studies of Children ...................................................................... 17 Studies of Adults ........................................................................ 20 Implementation of the Functional Approach ...................................... 21 Research Questions ............................................................................. 23 CHAPTER 2 METHODS ......................................................................................... 24 Participants ........................................................................................ 24 Description of Participants ............................................................ 24 Participant Selection Criteria.................... ..................................... 24 Participant Selection Procedures ....................... ' ............................ 25 Description of the Experimental Procedures .............................................. 26 Criteria for Task Selection ............................................................ 26 Task Description ........................................ . ............................... 27 Experimental CondItIons ............... 29 Data Collection Procedures29 Description of Task Administration to Experimental Groups ................. 30 Post-Task Survey ................................................................................ 32 Recording of Data ............................................................................... 33 Description of Performance Measures ..................................................... 33 Non-verbal Measure.................... ............................................... 33 Verbal Measure ......................................................................... 34 Scoring of Data ............... . .................................................................... 34 Reliability of Judgments ............................... 35 Statistical Analysis of Data .................................................................... 36 CHAPTER 3 RESULTS .......................................................................................... 37 Quantitative Data Analyses .............................................. - ...................... 3 7 Verbal Performance .............................................................................. 37 Between Groups .................................. ' ...................................... 39 Within Groups ............................................................................ 4O Nonverbal Performance ........................................................................ 40 Between Group Effects ................................................................ 41 Within Group Effects .................................. 43 Qualitative Data Analysis....‘ .................................................................. 43 Conclusion ......................................................................................... 47 CHAPTER 4 DISCUSSION ..................................................................................... 48 Interpretation ofVerbal Effects ............................................... 50 The Auditory-Verbal Modality Effect ................................................ 50 Tactual versus Visual Modality Effect .............................................. 53 Interpretation of Nonverbal Effects ........................................................... 58 Auditory-Verbal Modality Effects .................................................... 58 Visual and Tactual Modality Effects ..... , ........................................... 59 Implications for Further Research ............................................................ 61 Methodological Limitations in the Present Study ................................ 62 Theoretical Issues ....................................................................... 66 Clinical Implications .............................................................................. 67 Conclusion ......................................................................................... 7O APPENDICES ‘ Appendix A - Participant Characteristics ................................................... 72 Appendix B - Participant Recruitment Flier ........... . ................................... 74 Appendix C - Participant RecruitmentFlier for COM 100 Students. ........ 75 Appendix D - Participant Background Information ...................................... 76 I Appendix E - Approval to use Human Subjects .......................................... 77 Appendix F — Participant Consent Form .................................................... 78 Appendix G - Steps to Origami Task.................... .................................... 81 Appendix H — Pro-Experiment Demonstration of Vocabulary ......................... 82 Appendix I -— Auditory Group Instructions ................................................... 83 Appendix J - Auditory and Visual Group Instructions ................................ 84 Appendix K - Auditory, Visual, and Tactual Group Instructions ................... 85 Appendix L - Data Collection Sheet ...................................................... 86 Appendix M - Comet Information Unit Calculation Guidelines .................... 87 BIBLIOGRAPHY ............................................................................... 95 ' LIST OF TABLES Table 1 -Steps to origami task .......... . ...................... ...... 28 Table 2 - Group outcomes on verbal performance ................................. 38 Table 3 — Groupoutcomes on nonverbal performance ............................ 41 V Table 4 — Participants' verbatim responses for making the task easier ....... 44 Table 5 — Participants' reports of personal learning type .......... I ............... 46 vii Chapter One INTRODUCTION TO PROBLEM FOR STUDY The current study is motivated by the clinical issues that are related to the delivery of therapy services to patients with traumatic brain injury (TBI). TBI is broadly defined as brain injury due to externally inflicted trauma. If the trauma results in the skull with perforation of the meninges, the injury is considered to be an open-head injury. If the skull and the meninges remain intact, the injury is considered to be a closed-head injury (JAMA, 1999; Brookshire, 1997). Characteristics of TBI Incidence of TBI It is estimated that nearly 7 million people in the United States incur TBIs each year (Rosenthai, 1990). Traumatic brain injuries usually are associated with motor vehicle accidents, falls, acts of violence, and sports injuries. “Males are more likely than females to receive TBIs. For 20-year- olds, the ratio is approximately 4:1 male/female (Brookshire, 1997, p. 326).” Every year the frequency of TBI occurrence increases. Due to the numerous negative consequences of TBls for physical, psychological and social functioning, treatment of these individuals is an important issue for the health care system (Rosenthal, 1990). A TBI affects not only young adults. Individuals of all ages are at risk for sustaining a TBI. Young children (toddlers) and older adults (over 50 years of age) are at less risk than young adults, but they are still more likely to incur a TBI than the general population (Brookshire, 1997). The injuries sustained by young children and older adults are usually a result of a fall, whereas in the young adult population, the injuries are usually a result of motor vehicle accidents (Brookshire, 1997). While it is possible for anyone to sustain a traumatic brain injury, there are certain risk factors that make the probability greater for some individuals. These risk factors include: the use of alcohol and drugs, school adjustment and social history, socioeconomic status, personality type, history of TBI, and participation in sporting activity (Brookshire, 1997; Rosenthal, 1990). Differential Diagnosis: ghasia versus TBI Some may question the need to investigate intervention approaches and strategies for individuals with a traumatic brain injury. After all, clinicians have been working with aphasic individuals for decades. Aphasic individuals too have sustained injury to the brain. How then are individuals with aphasia different from those individuals who have sustained a TBI? It has been well documented that in fact, there is a difference between the two types of injuries (Freund, 1994; Chapey, 1994; Rosenthal, 1990; Gillis, 1996). If aphasic intervention approaches were sufficient to serve the TBI population, there would be no need for further research in this area. However, because the presenting deficits from the injury types differ, perhaps the treatment should also differ. Smptoms of TBI A broad range of symptoms can accompany a TBI. Physical impairments, such as limited use of limbs, may result from TBI, although the more problematic consequences involve the individual’s cognition, emotional functioning, and behavior (JAMA, 1999). According to Ben-Yishay (1983), “it has been established with virtual certainty that the long-term mental sequelae outstrip the physical impairments as a cause of difficulties with the vocational and personal rehabilitation of the patient...” (Ben-Yishay, 1983, p. 171). Some of the most commonly reported neurobehavioral sequelae associated with TBI include impaired memory, impaired attention and concentration, slowed information processing, decreased initiation, planning, and organization, inefficient word recall, tangentiality, diminished self-awareness/self-appraisal, talkativeness, irritability and restlessness, egocentricity, impulsivity, disinhibition, dependency, apathy, emotional lability, and depression (Gillis, 1996). In addition, frontal lobe functions, such as the executive skills of problem solving, abstract reasoning, insight, and judgment also may be affected when an individual sustains a TBI (JAMA, 1999). Several studies have explored the different symptoms incurred by individuals with a TBI. For example, Gronwell and Wrightson (1981) studied a total of 91 patients in two related studies. All patients were between the ages of 17-30 years old and had simple head injuries. They looked at deficits of memory after simple closed head injury and the relation of residual memory impairment to the duration of post traumatic amnesia (PTA). In the first experiment, 71 subjects were given the Wechsler Memory Scale, the Paced Auditory Serial Addition Task (PASAT), and the Quick-Test, to get an approximate verbal IQ. Subjects were grouped by the duration of their PTA. Subjects in the third group, who had the longest period of PTA, showed a significant Wechsler Memory Quotient (MQ) difference from those in groups one and two. The analysis of the first study suggested different consequences of simple closed head injury. The first could be measured with the PASAT and seemed to be concerned with attention, concentration, and information processing capacity. The second consequence clearly involved Ieaming and memory. In the first experiment Ieaming and memory were weakly related to PTA; therefore a second experiment was conducted. In this experiment, 20 subjects were given the PASAT, the Selective Reminding Task and the Visual Sequential Memory subtest from the Illinois Test of Psycholinguistic Abilities. Thirty normal subjects were also given the two memory tests. The patient group scored significantly below the control group on the visual sequential memory test. Patients also differed from the controls in their ability to retrieve material from memory store. The results of the study suggest that closed head injury has at least three different effects on memory: (1.) a deficit in information processing, (2.) a reduced ability to place material into long-ten'n memory store, and (3.) a deficit in the ability to retrieve material from memory once it has been stored. In another study of symptoms following TBI, Cicerone and Wood (1987) observed one 20-year-old male who sustained a severe closed head injury in a motor vehicle accident four years earlier. This patient demonstrated impaired planning ability and poor self-control. The first two months of cognitive remediation focused on attention training to decrease distractibility. After this period, treatment focused on the patient’s planning disorder. This treatment was based on a modified self-instructional training procedure that required the patient to verbalize a plan before and during execution of the training task and then to gradually fade overt verbalizations. Before the self-instructional training began, a baseline measure for a therapy task was attained. During this baseline the patient exhibited five classes of target behaviors. These behaviors included 1) self-stimulating verbalizations (e.g. whistling, singing) during task performance; 2) off-task verbalizations (e.g. talking about the weather); 3) off-task behaviors; 4) unplanned moves; and 5) incorrect moves. During the first stage of training there was a 91% decrease in the number of incorrect moves. Problem solving errors were reduced when the subjects stated the inappropriate behaviors aloud. Other target behaviors also showed a systematic reduction over the course of treatment. This treatment found that when the subjects began to internalize the self-verbalization strategy, off task behaviors were minimized. Unlike the focal lesions that often result in aphasia, traumatic brain injuries result in diffuse neurological damage. This is the reason why so many different cognitive processes are affected in TBI patients. While a cerebrovascular accident (CVA) may leave an individual impaired linguistically in the areas of syntax or phonology, a TBI is more likely to affect an individual’s use of language or pragmatics (Freund, 1994; Chapey, 1994). Clients with TBI may have language problems, but these problems are usually the result of generalized cognitive deficits and are qualitatively different from those seen in aphasia. The assessment and treatment principles of aphasia are based on the premise that most cognitive capacities are intact, whereas the assessment and treatment principles of TBI are based on the premise that many cognitive abilities are disturbed. (Freund, 1994, p. 15) Godfrey, Knight, Marsh, Moroney and Bishara (1989) described the pragmatic deficits of 18 TBI patients with closed head injuries (CHI). All CHI subjects had sustained the injury more than 18 months earlier. There was also a control group that consisted of 18 males with no history of neurological disorder. The control subjects were matched to the experimental group for age, years of education, and estimates of pre-morbid IQ. The investigators examined the relationship between social interaction and speed of information processing. All subjects took part in a self-assessment of social and emotional adjustment, a videotaped assessment of social behavior, and a neuropsychological evaluation. The results of the study showed that the CHI subjects had an information processing impairment compared to the normal controls. However, the study did not find that the slower information processing of the CHI subjects was related to their decreased social skills. It has been well supported that traumatic brain injuries disrupt several cognitive processes (Rosenthal, 1990; Freund, 1994; Levin, 1992; Gillis, 1996; Brooks, 1984). Due do these disruptions, both verbal and nonverbal performances are affected in individuals who incur traumatic brain injuries. According to the condensed version of Prigatano’s classification of cognitive deficits, there are six main areas of disturbance These include 1.) attention and concentration, 2.) initiation and goal direction, 3.) judgment and perception, 4.)leaming and memory, 5.) speed of information processing, and 6.) communication (Rosenthal, 1990). While any combination of these disturbances can be detrimental to the way an individual functions, the most practical nonverbal processes affected are those required to complete activities of daily living (ADLs) (Rosenthal, 1990). One’s ability to perform ADLs may also be affected if/when the nonverbal processes are taxed to the point where an individual is unable to solve problems of daily living independently (Rosenthal, 1990; Gillis, 1996). Many activities that head injured patients have difficulty performing include, but are not limited to feeding, dressing, personal hygiene skills, transfers, homemaking skills, time management, financial management, and driving (Rosenthal, 1990; Gillis, 1996). The above cited studies of the literature makes clear that people with TBIs sustain a range of cognitive deficits that affect both verbal and nonverbal performances. The question at hand now, is what is the most effective way to intervene with these patients? This is an especially critical question to ask for speech-language rehabilitation. This is because speech-language pathologists have been trained to think primarily about the verbal performances of their patients. Because TBIs affect both verbal and nonverbal abilities, intervention that effectively meets patients’ needs must also influence nonverbal abilities. To do this, the foundation for therapy should be cognitively based, in ways that take into account both linguistic and non- linguistic domains of function. (Rosenthal, 1990; Green, 1997; Kreutzer, 1991). Intervention Persgctives Broadly speaking there are two types of competing therapy approaches in use today for working with individuals with TBls: the neuropsychological approach, or process specific approach, and the functional approach. Neuropsycholggical Approach The neuropsychological approach is driven by the assumption that the patient’s overall cognitive functioning will improve by targeting specific cognitive areas or functions for remediation (Levin, 1992; Sohlberg & Mateer, 1989; Gillis, 1996). Examples of specific cognitive processes that might be addressed include: attention, memory, thought organization, and information processing (Green, 1997). Sohlberg and Mateer (1989) claim that there are six fundamental rehabilitation principles that characterize this therapy approach: 1. A theoretically motivated model defines each cognitive process area. 2. Therapy tasks are administered repetitively. 3. Goals and objectives are hierarchically organized. 4. Remediation involves data-based and directed treatment. 5. The use of generalization probes provides measurements of treatment success. 6. Ultimate measures of success must be improvements in level of vocational ability and independent living. To implement or apply the above principles however, tasks are often administered repetitively. These are usually artificial tasks that are isolated from real-life events. For example, one therapy activity may be alternating between sorting large and small paper clips and sorting colored rubber bands (Green, 1997). While the underlying goal of the activity (i.e., alternating attention) is to benefit to the patient’s ability to function in daily life, this type of activity does not show the patient how to integrate the skill from the isolated therapy task into a functional task done within a daily living context. It is assumed that the patient will be able to integrate the skill into a functional task on his or her own. Unfortunately, this may not happen for some patients, especially those who are severely impaired. In fact, clinical observations commonly reveal that performance in sanitized test situations and improvements in performance in isolated cognitive processes and systems do not automatically transfer to more functional tasks in real-world contexts.(Ylvisaker, 1998, p. 136) Functional Approach Whereas the neuropsychological or process-specific therapy approach is popular for individuals with TBIs, functional therapy is another approach commonly used with this population (Gillis, 1996; Gruen, 1994;Kreutzer, 1991; Rosenthal, 1990). Unlike the neuropsychological approach, the functional approach: is not limited to skills training in and of itself. Cognitive processes may be addressed at a component level, but the context in which they are addressed must take into account the eventual expected activities of the individual. (Gillis, 1996, p. 147). This approach requires the patient to integrate the skill(s) in a functional task. By taking this approach, the patient receives a rich experience in that both verbal and nonverbal stimulation are received. This approach may be especially relevant to patients with severe impairment because it may help them to integrate skills in tasks, thereby making the task environmentally relevant to the patient. While the functional therapy approach is being described more frequently in the literature, there are at least two issues that need to be addressed before discussing why it is being examined in the current study. What is meant by functional, and what kind of support exists for its use in a treatment protocol? What is functional? Unfortunately, the term “functional” has not been clearly defined. But it is important to know how “functional" has been defined by those using such a functional approach to rehabilitation. Proponents of the functional approach may focus on a list of functional activities or tasks (e.g., listening to a story about a hypothetical situation and then responding to questions about the situation (Gruen, 1994). This is viewed as functional because these are situations that could present themselves in everyday life. While the verbal treatment material is about functional events, no assumption is being made about how the patients are to solve the problems in the actual situation. They simply answer the questions. The only input the patients 10 receive is auditory verbal. The patients are not “doing” anything during these tasks. During these functional tasks, attention is not given to how the patient should do these tasks. That is, how a task is done is often not a focus of therapy. Yet the act of doing is implicitly functional. Too often in therapy, the nonverbal performance on a task is considered as the output or product of the Ieamed task (i.e., a motor act) and not as a basis for Ieaming. Instead, it is proposed here that the motor act of “doing" should be viewed as another source of sensory input for the patient. This study is broadly motivated by the need to Ieam more about the functional therapy approach as it may be the preferred mode of treatment for severe patients. For the purpose of this study, the definition of functional is an actual-hands on experience that affects the next sequence of events. Rationale for the Present Study It is not enough to just state that doing tasks as part of therapy is beneficial. Two perspectives, which support the functional approach to therapy, will be reviewed here. These perspectives provide the theoretical justification for the present study. Event Knowledge as the Root of Intelligent Behavior Nelson (1986) argued that event representation is a basic foundation for both verbal and nonverbal Ieaming. In defining an event, Nelson (1986) stated that events are about objects and their relationship to the environment. She claims that events are not static, but that they take place through time. Stockman (2000) explains events as interactions within the environment that provide multiple forms of sensory input, including tactual-kinesthetic input. This input then provides feedback about the cause-effect relationship that actions have on the environment. The hypothesis on which much of Nelson’s work is based is “ that the content and structure of knowledge representation is an important source of observed changes in cognitive functioning” (Nelson, 1986,p.3) Nelson’s theory fits well with the notion of providing input to a TBI patient via the act of doing an event. She argues that event knowledge is central to cognition in young children and adults (Nelson, 1986). By this she means that children obtain knowledge about the world through direct experiences with interacting with the environment. This is because they do not have the same access to new information as older children and adults (Nelson, 1986). Children cannot Ieam the same way through books, oral instructions, and television because they do not have the same knowledge of language as adults and older children (Nelson, 1986). Nelson discusses the importance of scripts, and how they influence a child’s language development. A script is defined as a particular set of action steps that must take place in a designated sequence in order to achieve a desired outcome. Verbal and nonverbal scripts for behaving are derived from routine daily life events (e.g., eating, dressing, etc.). That is, children Ieam scripts from the routine events of daily living events, which are structured by adult caregivers. These scripts allow children to create categories for word meanings and I7 associations with events. Scripts do not pertain to children alone. Nelson (1996) stated that With respect to the function of scripts, research with adults has demonstrated that the event schemas individuals bring to a situation influence their interpretation of new experiences, their understanding of stories and ability to cany on discourse, and enable them to form plans and make inferences and predictions. (Nelson, 1986, p. 15) This relates to TBI patients. Depending on the level of severity, they too may have difficulty accessing the knowledge of language that they once possessed before the injury. Consequently, they would have to regain this knowledge or at least access to it from direct experience with events. This is not to say that a patient who sustains a TBI has the same level of functioning as a child, but rather the assumption is that a brain-damaged individual may revert to earlier forms of Ieaming. Therefore, the individual’s knowledge can be reorganized by appealing to the same processes that are needed for children to develop new knowledge. Nelson is not the only theorist who supports the idea of “doing” as a fundamental aspect of organized perceptual cognitive representation of experiences. Affolter and Bischofberger (2000) also support doing as an activity providing valuable sensory input to an individual. Tactile experience is considered the most critical form of input for providing the root of development for problem solving in one’s environment (Affolter 8r Bischofberger, 2000). I? Affolter and Bischofberger: The Model of a Root and Learning Affolter and Bischofberger (2000) provide a new model of development that Is rooted by tactile-kinesthetic input. They use the root of a growing tree and its relationship to the growing branches as a metaphor for describing how all Ieamers acquire new knowledge. Affolter and Bischofberger (2000) state: The growth of a tree depends on the growth of its root. When the root is small as in a young tree, only a few branches are visible. The stronger the root gets, the stronger the trunk becomes, and the more branches that will grow. When the root gets sick, the branches get sick as a consequence. The branches are not related directly to each other; rather they are related to the root. So their relationship is an indirect one. Branches represent the levels of development. The growth of a branch represents the appearance of a given level with its respective skills. Just as the branches are indirectly related, so are the levels of development. They are indirectly related through the root. (Affolter & Bischofberger, 2000, p. 142) The focus now is, how do Affolter and Bischofberger (2000) relate this to individuals Ieaming new tasks? They state that the root is responsible for the growth of the tree. In this analogy, the root represents basic human experience (i.e., nonverbal interaction/experience in daily life events). Affolter and Bischofberger define nonverbal interaction in daily life as “events with goal-oriented changes in topological relationships between a persons body engaged in an event and the environment” (Affolter & Bischofberger, 2000, p. 142). This interaction inherently results in causes and effects (i.e., cognitive processes). Affolter and Bischofberger (2000) claim that as the child experiences more nonverbal daily interactions with the environment, there is an improvement in the perceptual organization and cognitive processes. Affolter and Bischofberger also argue, IA that the basic kind of nonverbal interaction experience, which is represented by the root, requires tactual information. Although vision and hearing seems to enhance nonverbal interaction activity. (Affolter 8r Bischofberger, 2000, p. 145) Affolter and Bischofberger (2000) claim that their model of development and Ieaming is not only valid for children, it is also valid for adults. They support this claim with their study of babies and adult patients with brain damage (Affolter, Bischofberger, 8r Calabretti, 1996). The purpose of their study was to increase the understanding of brain-damaged patients’ nonverbal daily interaction deficits by comparing these patients to normal babies interact with the environment. The participants were 54 normal babies, ages 0-18 months, and 20 adult patients with TBI, ages 18-70. The babies were observed in daily living situations such as diaper changing, feeding, and play. The adults were also observed in daily living situations such as meals, group activities, and helping in daily needs of the rehabilitation center. The results of their study showed that the brain-damaged patients showed a level of complexity of nonverbal interaction units in daily life activities that were similar to that of babies. This then suggests that the deficits of brain-damaged individuals can help to be described by what is known about developmental data. Theoretical Assumptions of Affolter and Bischofberger’s Model Affolter and Bischofberger came to their model based on four principle assumptions as summarized by Stockman (2000): 1.) Nonverbal interaction with the physical environment in daily problem- solving events is the root of development; 2.) Nonverbal interaction is critically dependent on perceiving information that is anchored by the 1C tactual-kinesthetic input associated with elicited changes of topological relationships between the body and the environment; 3.) Inadequate nonverbal interaction experiences hinder development of nonverbal and verbal skills when a central nervous system deficit prevents perceptual activity from being organized well enough to search for the relevant information; 4.) Inadequate nonverbal interaction experiences can be impoverished by a variety of central perceptual deficits among children with language disorders, who are not a homogenous clinical group. (Affolter & Bischofberger, 2000, p. 197) Statement of the Problem While the previous paragraphs have focused on the importance of the functional approach to therapy, it still remains less well studied than the more traditional neuropsychological therapy approach. More information is needed about the functional therapy approach as it may prove beneficial to severe patients who have the most trouble with activities of daily living (ADLs). Another reason is the theoretical argument that we need to function at a foundational level aimed at improving the underlying organization of perceptual-cognitive ability in TBI patients. To get that foundation, the , theoretical frameworks above Affolter and Bischofberger in particular, stress the representation of functional events in which persons participate by moving their bodies to interact with the world around them. Such activity requires tactile-kinesthetic perception. It is assumed that the processes necessary for Ieaming a new task are basic to all Ieaming at all ages and are not applicable solely to traumatically brain injured adults, or any kind of injury. Therefore, these same processes are also applicable to all normal individuals as well, and especially for adults Ieaming a new task. This is not to say that mature Ieamers do not Ieam tasks from just auditory-verbal and/or visual input, but that when a task is new, it may be necessary for them to actually do a new task in order to lay the foundation for comprehending the associated auditory and visual experiences in the absence of an action event. This argument justifies the study of normal adult Ieamers in the present study. Studies of normal adult Ieamers can lay the groundwork for future studies of clinical populations such as TBI. Studies of normal adult Ieamers also are important to do because up to now, children have been typically the focus of research aimed at determining the effect of “hands-on” experience on Ieaming. The following sections illustrate how some of these studies have been done with either children or adults. Studies of Children Stockman and Latham (2001) observed 15 normal children with the mean age being 5; 1 yrs. Stockman and Latham investigated whether the verbal and nonverbal performances of typically developing children who participated in a novel event would differ from those who only observed the events visually. The children Ieamed to do a novel juice-making task. They were randomly assigned to either an observation group or a participation group. The observation group listened to and watched a videotape of the examiner making juice. The participation group was manually guided by the same examiner to do the task. During the task they heard two nonsense words. One word represented the action of pressing down on the juicer and the other word represented the novel object - the antique juicer. 17 The children were then judged on two different measures, a verbal measure and a nonverbal measure. Performance on the verbal measures revealed that the children in the auditory/visual group correctly associated the novel word with the object more often than the participation group; however, the participation group associated the novel action word with the action more often than the observation group. A significant difference was also found between the two groups in their verbal answers to questions about the task. The participation group scored significantly better than the observation group. For the nonverbal performance measure, the children had to reproduce the task on their own. The participation group scored significantly better than the observation group on this measure. Stockman and Latham (2001) concluded, that the verbal and nonverbal performances of the typically developing preschoolers were affected by the modality of sensory input. Latham and Stockman (2001) also examined whether the same results would be obtained from children with special needs. The group with special needs included children with a range of clinical diagnoses inclusive of those along the spectrum of pervasive developmental delay with a diagnosis of autism. But the children all exhibited clinically significant verbal deficits. The participants were randomly assigned to an auditory/visual observation group or to a participation group. The same nonverbal and verbal tasks and procedures were used as described above for typically developing children. special needs. 18 When the children’s verbal performance was measured, there was a significantly higher score for the participation group than the observation group. However, the nonverbal performance measure showed no significant differences between the special needs observation and participation groups in their ability to reenact the nonverbal task. When compared to the normal children in the earlier study, it was clear that typically developing children performed better than the children with special needs on both verbal and nonverbal tasks. However, the special needs observation group performed the poorest of all groups. Latham and Stockman’s (2001) findings suggest “...that one cannot dismiss the relevance of hands-on experiences, especially for the special needs child” (Latham & Stockman, 2001 ). Another study that supported the hands-on approach to intervention was conducted by Glenburg (2002). Glenburg assessed reading comprehension and explored how it was affected by the manipulation of objects. The participants were 33 second grade children. Glenburg divided the students into three different experimental groups. One group was the indexical reading approach (IRA) group. Its participants were allowed to manipulate the objects about which they read. The second group was a non- manipulate group, and the third group was a classroom control group. All of the children had to read short stories, but just the children in the IRA group got to manipulate the objects whereas the children in the other groups did not. The results showed that the children in the IRA group were able to recall 73% of the sentences that they read, whereas the children in the non-manipulate group only recalled 49% of the sentences they had read. While the 2 previously described studies imply that there is a connection between tactile-kinesthetic input and performance measures of different kinds, these studies were done with children; one with normal children, one with special needs children, and one in which the focus of the study was reading comprehension. These studies provide reason to suggest that tactile-kinesthetic input may enhance the performance of adults. Studies of Adults The relevant research cited here was done with aphasic patients. Hinckley (1999) wanted to determine whether there are differential outcomes of functional and neuropsychological approaches to clinical treatment for naming deficits among adults with chronic nonfluent aphasia. She studied two groups with 12 adults each. They were comparable in age, post-onset time, socioeconomic status, and aphasia severity. Hinckley (1999) measured the subjects’ pre- and post-intervention performances using standardized measures of language comprehension, naming and functional communication. These adults also provided a discourse sample and participated in a specially designed functional task. This task required participants to role-play in a catalog-ordering scenario. After 20 hours of therapy for five weeks, subjects were re-evaluated. Hinckley (1999) found that those subjects that received functionally based treatment improved on both the trained task and on a task of oral naming. rm Hinckley also found that the cognitive neuropsychologically trained subjects improved on broad measures of language abilities, including standardized functional communication assessment and caregiver ratings of communication. Although this study examined the differences between the neuropsychological approach and the functional approach, it was done with aphasic patients. It is unknown whether such a difference would exist for patients with traumatic brain injury. Implementation of the Functional Approach Lastly, a main component of the functional approach in which more information is needed, is how therapists are to go about teaching the functional task. When teaching any task, the input becomes the focus for the patient and the clinician. This is because the input is all that the clinician has control over. Clinicians can decide and control what sensory channel the input is received in, and they can also decide the complexity level of the task. By default, the sensory channels of choice for clinicians through the years have usually been vision and audition. For example, when teaching a new task to a patient, the clinician may say, “I will now tell you how the task is done,” or the clinician may say, “okay, now watch how I do this,” or finally, the clinician may say, “now watch me and do the task at the same time I do it.” While the third approach stated includes the act of “doing” the task, it often is not the modality most stressed. Instead, it is believed that the success comes from the visual input alone and not the collaboration between the two senses. Some may ask, why the tactile-kinesthetic system is important to Ieaming? The answer is because it helps one Ieam to know something by involving his/her body in a direct way. If we look back at the premise of Affolter and Bischofberger’s (2000), we see that in their framework, function is defined as “events with goal-oriented changes in topological relationships between a person who interacts and the environment” (Affolter & Bischofberger, 2000, p. 142). Affolter and Bischofberger (2000) have studied patients in hospitals and clinics all over Europe in order to help them develop their model of development and Ieaming. However, more research needs to be done in this area of both atypical and typical populations because much of Affolter and Bischofberger's argument has been based on theory and indirect empirical evidence from existing research. One goal of this study is to test one assumption of Affolter and Bischofberger’s model, and that is the importance of tactile-kinesthetic input. In particular, this study will provide empirical data about whether complex Ieaming improves when it involves tactile-kinesthetic input. Even though the tactile-kinesthetic system is assumed to provide useful information, little is known about this system. If this system is, in fact fundamentally important to human perception and memory involved in doing functional tasks, then it should be reflected in normal persons/Ieamers. Unfortunately, when asking what is known about normal Ieaming in this regards, it turns out that little is known about this either. This is because the ’7’) same biases toward using distance senses for Ieaming and releaming permeate the literature on normal Ieamers as well. With normal Ieaming experiments, it is easy to rely on these distance senses because these persons are assumed to have already stored a rich or elaborate perceptual- cognitive organization in which distance sensory inputs are connected to body senses. However, for novel tasks, we might be able to observe a greater reliance on actual “hands-on” input. Research Questions The purpose of this study was to examine whether the verbal and nonverbal performances of normal adult males on a novel procedural task were differentially affected by: 1) the sensory modality of input, and 2) the time delay for displaying Ieaming. 23 Chapter Two METHODS Participants Description of Participants There were 21 participants in the study. They all met the following criteria: They were male Caucasians, between the ages of 18 and 21. All were Michigan State University students with normal hearing and no self-reported history of brain damage or Ieaming disability and had normal or corrected to normal vision. This information, obtained from participant self-report, is summarized in Appendix A. Although 24 persons responded to the request to participate in the study, 3 of them were excluded because they did not meet the eligibility requirements. Two of these individuals were female, and one was not within the pre-specified age range for the study. Due to time constraints for completing the project, more participants were not pursued. Participant Selection Criteria Twenty-four participants were initially sought to participate in this study. All participants had to be male, college students, between the ages of 18 and 21 years. In addition, all participants had to have normal hearing, no history of brain damage or Ieaming disability, and normal or corrected to normal vision. An additional selection criterion was that none of the participants could have any previous experience with tasks related to the task in the study, i.e., origami. This criterion was important to ensuring that the participants would Ieam a novel task. The number of participants had been decided upon based on practical time constraints for completing the project. The choice of 24 participants was expected to be minimally adequate for statistical analysis, given the exploratory pilot nature of this study. Participant Selection Procedures want recruiLrnent procedppep. Participants for the study were recruited using fliers posted at various sites on Michigan State University’s campus and through the Communication 100 (COM100) class participant pool through Michigan State University (see Appendix B and C). The recruitment notice stated that Caucasian, males between the ages of 18 and 21 were being sought for a research project to be done in the Communication Arts and Sciences building. The recruitment notice also contained contact information for reaching the examiner. Contact information included the examiner’s name, email address, and home telephone number. The flier clearly stated that the participants in the study would be reimbursed for their time at a rate of $12.00 an hour or they could choose to receive class credit for COM 100 if they were enrolled in that course. Once the participants contacted the examiner, they undenNent a further level of screening. P_artickfint screening procedures. During the screening process the participants were asked to provide a written self-report of basic identification information that included: their name, the ethnic group they identified with, their age, their major, whether there was a history of brain damage or Ieaming disability, if their hearing was within normal limits and if their vision was within normal limits or corrected to within normal limits. In addition, the participants were also asked about previous experiences with origami (see Appendix D). Human participant assurances. The participants in this study were not endangered at any time. The procedure was designed to ensure the participants’ safety during the experiment, and it had been approved by the University Committee on Research Involving Human Subjects (see Appendix E). All information obtained from the participants has been kept confidential by assigning each participant a number that was used for purposes of data analysis. All participants provided informed written consent before beginning the research study (see Appendix F). The participants were informed that their participation was completely voluntary and they could withdraw from the study at anytime. Description of the Exgrimental Procedures Criteria for Task Selection Before describing what the experimental task was, I should explain what type of task was selected. The examiner wanted to select a novel task that would be challenging to normal Ieamers with a lot of prior everyday problem-solving experiences. It also had to be possible to Ieam the task using either one or a combination of sensory modalities within a reasonable amount of time. In addition, the task had to be a procedural task. The reason for choosing a procedural task was that its step-like approach for completion could correspond to problem solving tasks in the work place. Performance of job tasks is an area of concern for individuals with Traumatic Brain Injuries (TBls). As mentioned in the previous chapter, many such individuals (i.e., people with TBls) are often young adults and of working age. So any treatment must be geared toward helping them to return to work or school successfully. Task description Taking all of the above criteria into account, the task chosen for this experiment was a paper-folding task, more specifically, an origami task. The word origami comes from the Japanese language and means folded paper. Because the difficulty level can vary, this type of task meets all of the task selection criteria. For this study, the task of an origami card case with a difficulty level of very easy was chosen. The level of very easy represented the judgment of one author of a book about origami. The task used in this study was taken from the book mga_mi (Gross, 2001). Although the task used was judged to be very easy, it should be noted that to someone unfamiliar with origami the task of making an origami card case would present a challenge. The steps required for the task are shown in Table 1. 0'7 Table 1. Steps to Origpmi Tfik Steps to Origami Task 1. Position the paper vertically in front of you. 2. Fold the paper in half, bringing the top to the bottom and crease. 3. Take the top flap and fold the paper up to the crease and crease the fold. 4. Unfold 5. Turn the paper over. 6. Take the top flap and fold the paper up to the crease and crease the fold. 7. Unfold 8. Take the bottom left comer and fold it up to the crease in the middle of the page. 9. 14. 15. 16. 17. Repeat the previous step with the remaining three corners. 10. Fold the top flap along the middle crease. 11 .Turn the paper over. 12.Fold the top flap along the middle crease. 13. Take the top flap and fold it up, creating a rectangle with a flat surface. Turn the paper vertically. Make a one-inch fold at the top of the paper, creating two pockets. Insert the bottom comers into the pockets. Fold the paper in half, bringing the left side over to the right. These same steps were provided verbatim to the participants (see Appendix G). The paper used in this origami task was a type of paper typically used for charcoal drawings. The sheets were 9”x12” with a weight of 95 g/m2. The sheets were off-white in color and had a textured surface. A textured surface was used because it was judged to provide more tactual input than smooth paper. Exmrimental Conditions Participants were randomly assigned to one of three experimental groups. The participants of each group Ieamed the experimental task under one of three input conditions. These included: 1.) Auditory input, 2.) Auditory and visual input, and 3.) Auditory, visual and tactual input. Having received information about how to do the task in the Ieaming phase of the experiment, participants’ retention was assessed immediately after completion of the initial instructions and after a 20-minute time interval. During this time interval the participants engaged in a distraction task, which required them to search for hidden words. Data Collection Procedures Administration conditions. The data were collected in a laboratory-like setting at Michigan State University. The room was part of the early language laboratory located in the basement of the Communication Arts and Sciences building. The data collection took place on three separate occasions over a time period of one month. Participants from each group were randomly tested on each of the three separate data collection occasions. Instructions for doing the task. All of the participants received instructions for the task in the same room. Only one participant was present at a time. Before beginning the experiment, each participant had to demonstrate understanding of vocabulary that was to be used in the study (see Appendix H). Each participant also received specific instructions, which were provided 29 both auditorily and visually, as to what he was to do for the task (See Appendix I, J, K). Description of Task Administration to Exgrimental Groups A detailed explanation of how the task was administered to each of the three test groups follows. m. This group received instructions via auditory-verbal input. The auditory input was standardized across participants by using a pre-recorded audio tape of a female voice reading the steps to complete the task. The words that were used on the tape are the same words that can be found in Appendix G. After a step was read, there was a two second pause, and then the same step was repeated two more times. Each time the repeated presentation was separated by a two second pause. This ensured that the participants in this experimental group were offered as much opportunity to experience as much input as the participants in the other input conditions. After the participants listened once to all of the instructions, the examiner presented the task again in the same manner used for the first presentation. After the second presentation of the steps for the task, the participants attempted to perform the task by themselves with no further input from the examiner. After this performance, participants were asked to provide procedural discourse, or in other words, describe how they would instruct someone else on how to complete the task they had just done. After that, the participants were escorted into another room where they took part in a word search task for 20 minutes. This distraction task was chosen because it was assumed to be unrelated to the experimental task at hand. By participating in the distraction task, subjects were not able to practice the experimental task in the time before demonstrating what they had Ieamed. Following the 20 minutes, the participants returned to the original room to attempt the task again. And again, after performing the task, or attempting to, the participants had to provide procedural discourse. G_ro_pp_2_. The participants received instructions via auditory-verbal input as described above; in addition, they also received visual input. The examiner and the subjects (one at a time) were seated side by side at a table. The examiner performed the task as the participant looked on. This group received the same tape- recorded instructions that the auditory-verbal group received. After an auditory-verbal instruction was given, however, the participants in this group also saw the step performed by the examiner. After all of the steps for the task were given to the participants, the process was repeated again in the same manner as the first presentation. After the second presentation of auditory and visual input about how to do the task, the participants attempted the task on their own with no further input from the examiner. This group also had to provide procedural discourse. After that, the participants were escorted to another room just like group one and they took part in the same distraction task. Also like group one, after 20 minutes, the participants returned to the original room to perform both the verbal and non-verbal components of the task. M. This group received instructions via auditory-verbal, visual, and tactile input. This group listened to the same tape-recorded message as the two previously described groups. The participants in this group also received the visual input from the examiner seated next to them, but instead of just watching the examiner demonstrate each step, the participants in this group were asked to perform the steps at the same time the examiner was demonstrating them. After all of the steps were shown to the participants, the instructions were repeated in the same manner as the first presentation. After the second presentation of auditory, visual and tactual input about how to do the task, the participants attempted the task on their own with no further input from the examiner. This group, like both groups previously described, also provided procedural discourse. After doing so the participants were then escorted to another room to take part in a 20-minute distraction task. Following the 20 minutes, the participants returned to the original room to again perform both the verbal and non-verbal components of the task. Post-Task Survey After the participants engaged in the experimental task for the second time (i.e., following the distraction task), they were asked to orally respond to three questions about the task. The first question required them to rate on a scale of 1 to 5 how comfortable they were in their ability to correctly complete the task. On a scale of 1 — 5, a rating of corresponded to the least comfortable and 5 to the most comfortable. The participants were also asked to judge 17 whether they were more of an auditory, visual, or hands-on learner. This information was used during the data analysis to see if a bias existed in how the participants performed relative to the testing condition in which they were placed. Recording of Data It was necessary to document the participants’ verbal and nonverbal responses to the experimental task for data analyses. Each participant had a data record form with his identification number on it (see Appendix L). This form identified which experimental group the participant was in and whether he was able to complete the nonverbal performance measure. The participants’ responses to the post-task survey also were recorded on the same form. A cameraman was present to record all participants’ performances of the verbal and non-verbal measures. This was done with a Sony digital camera model number DCR-TRV140 NTSC. In addition, the procedural discourse was audio taped with a Panasonic mini cassette recorder model number RQ-L10. Description of Performance Measures Non-verbal Measure The participants were judged by two different measures. The first was their ability to non-verbally re-produce the paper-folding task on their own. This nonverbal portion of the task was assessed right after the task training input in order to observe immediate recall. This same portion was also assessed after a 20-minute delay in order to observe the participants' retention N of the task. The non-verbal performance was measured in terms of the number of correct steps of the task that each participant was able to complete. Verbal Measure The second measure of the participants’ performance was their ability to describe the procedure for doing the task. Again, the participants were asked to discuss the procedures for completing the task immediately after task training, and after a 20—minute delay. The verbal performance, i.e., the procedural discourse, was evaluated in terms of total number of correct information units (ClUs) given by each participant (Nicholas & Brookshire, 1993 as cited in Hinckley, 1999) (See Appendix M). The system of identifying ClUs is a “rule-based system for quantifying the inforrnativeness of connected speech elicited with a variety of stimuli” (Nicholas 8 Brookshire, 1993). Scoring of Data This investigator scored the nonverbal measure, or the number of correct steps that the participants were able to perform. After this was done someone else scored a portion of the nonverbal data for reliability purposes. This investigator also orthographically transcribed all of the procedural discourse provided by the participants. These transcripts were then distributed to three certified speech-language pathologists for scoring the verbal measure. These clinicians first counted the total number of words provided by each participant. Then they counted how many of these words 14 qualified as ClUs. I This was done according to the rules for scoring and counting words and ClUs provided by Nicholas and Brookshire (1993). Reliability of Judgments As mentioned above, after the examiner scored the number of correct steps that each participant was able to complete, another judge, a Michigan State University engineering graduate, independently judged a portion of the nonverbal data. A point-to-point comparison of the judgments yielded a 97% agreement for the nonverbal task. As mentioned above, the participants' verbal responses were randomly assigned to three judges in equal number. Each judge received responses from participants in each of the three experimental groups, but was blind to which group each participant belonged. To assess the reliability of these judgments, each judge, on a different occasion, re-scored a portion of the data she had previously scored in order to obtain a measure of self-agreement. A significant difference between how the judges scored the data from time 1 to time 2 was not found (t = 0.96, p > 0.05). Inter-judge reliability was assessed to determine the extent with which the judges agreed amongst themselves. To do this, each judge took a portion of the data previously scored by each of the other two judges and re-scored it. While there were some differences among judges noted, there was no significant difference found (t = 0.77, p > 0.05). Statistical Analfiis of Data The student Minitab (Schaefer, R.L., & Anderson, RB, 1987) Software was used for the statistical analysis of the data. This program provided measures of central tendencies as well as Type One error probability. Comparisons between the experimental groups were also made using a two- sample t-test. ’26 Chapter Three RESULTS In this study, participants were taught a novel, procedural task under one of three sensory input modality conditions. Participants received either auditory input only, or they received both auditory-verbal and visual input, or auditory-verbal plus visual and tactual input. After Ieaming the task, they had to demonstrate their Ieaming by performing the task and by providing verbal procedural discourse about the task. This demonstration of Ieaming took place immediately following the task instructions and after a 20-minute time delay Quantitative Data Analfies Given the exploratory nature of this study and the small number of participants observed, statistical analyses of the data were guided by the use of a liberal alpha level for rejecting the null hypothesis (p = .10 or less). This less stringent approach allowed the investigator to maximally exploit the data for trends that could be more rigorously tested in future research. Verbal Performance Table 2 on page 45 represents the between-group and within-group differences in the participants’ verbal performances (i.e., the procedural discourse provided by the participants) before and after a 20-minute time delay. From the procedural discourse, the number of correct information units (ClUs) was determined (see criteria in Appendix M). The number of CNS for ’27 each participant in each group was obtained. The 3 experimental groups were compared on their mean number of ClUs using a between group one-way analysis of variance. The 3 treatment groups means were statistically compared using Tukey’s post-hoc statistic. Each experimental group also was compared to itself. The mean number of ClUs produced by a particular group before and after the 20-minute time delay was calculated using a two-sample t statistic. Table 2. Group Outcomes gn Verbal Performance Time 1 Group 1 Group 2 Group 3 (Aud. Input) (Aud+ Vis Input) (Aud+Vis+Tact. Input) N 7 7 7 Mean ClUs 72.0 92.1 89.4 SD 32.3 47.3 41.1 E value E (df=2,18)= 0.50, p > 0.10 Time 2 Mean ClUs 81.4 81.4 122.7 SD 33.9 42.6 23.1 t_ score 053, p = 0.61 I0.44, p = 0.67 -1.86, p < 0.095 13 value E (df=2,18)= 3.40, p < 0.056 38 Between Groups Iim_e_1. As Table 1 shows at time 1, group 2, which received auditory and visual sensory input, produced the highest mean number of ClUs (M = 92.1, S_D = 47.3). Group 3 which received auditory, visual and tactual input had the next highest mean number of ClUs (M = 89.4, S_D = 41.1) and group 1, which received only auditory sensory input, produced the lowest mean number of ClUs (M = 72.0, SQ = 32.3). However, a one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) revealed no significant group main effect (F (df=2,18) = 0.91 p > 0.05) before the 20-minute time delay. M. Comparison of the mean number of ClUs produced by each group after the 20-minute time delay, revealed no significant difference between the group that received only auditory sensory input (M = 81.4, SQ = 33.9) and the group that received auditory and visual sensory input (M = 81.4, SD = 33.9). However, the group that received tactual sensory input in addition to the auditory and visual sensory input produced a significantly larger mean number of ClUs (M = 122.7, S_D = 23.1) than did either of the other groups. The one-way ANOVA revealed a significant group main effect (F (df=2,18) = 3.40 p < 0.05). Pair wise group comparisons of the experimental groups” CIU means using the Tukey statistic (g = 5.09) revealed that the group that received the tactual sensory input was significantly higher than the group that received only the auditory sensory input and the group that received the auditory and the visual sensory input; the latter two groups did not differ significantly from each other. 39 Within Groups In addition to examining between group differences, each group was compared to itself before and after the 20-minute time delay for displaying what had been Ieamed (see Table 1). The t-test analysis revealed no statistically significant difference before or after the time delay for any two individual groups compared. However, by using the liberal alpha level of .10, it can be seen that the tactual input group differed significantly from the other groups in the mean number of ClUs produced. This was the only group to show any measurable change from time 1 to time 2 (t = -1.86, p < 0.095). This outcome suggests that when given an opportunity to consolidate the sensory input over a period of time, even a brief amount such as 20-minutes, performance is enhanced by the additional sensory input offered by the tactual modality. Nonverbal Performance This analysis focused on the outcomes when the participants attempted to do the nonverbal or the origami task independently of any input from the investigator. Just 1 (14%) of the 7 participants in the auditory-verbal group completed the nonverbal origami task. In the auditory-visual group, 4 (57%) of the 7 participants did so, as did 5 or 71% if the 7 participants in the auditory- visuaI-tactual group. However, the main analysis focused on the number of steps performed correctly irrespective of task completion. 40 Table 3 shows the between group and within group differences for the participants’ nonverbal performances both before and after the 20-minute time delay. Table 3. Group Outcomes on Nonverbal Performance Time 1 Group 1 Group 2 Group 3 (Aud. Input) (Aud.+Vis. Input) (Aud.+Vis.+Tact. Input) N 7 7 7 Mean # of 4.8 13.8 14.0 Correct Steps SD 5.4 5.2 4.2 E (df=2,18)=7.65, p < 0.004 Time 2 Mean # of 4.8 12.4 14.1 Correct Steps SD 5.4 5.8 3.5 t score I0.00, p = 1.0 0.48, p = 0.64 -0.0, p= 0.95 E (dfi2,18)=6.70, p_ < 0.007 Between Group Effects As shown in table 3, the group that received just auditory sensory input performed the least number of correct steps to complete the nonverbal task (M = 4.8, _S_D_ = 5.4). The average number of steps completed by the group that received auditory and visual input and the group that received auditory, visual and tactual sensory input varied from M_= 13.8, S_D= 5.24 to M= 14.0, S_D= 4.2 respectively. The group that received the combined auditory, visual and 41 tactual sensory input completed the most steps on the average. The F test of differences among group means revealed a significant between groups effect (E = 7.6 (df = 2,18), p < 0.004) in the mean number of correct steps performed before the 20-minute time delay. Pair wise comparison of group means using the Tukey statistic (g = 11.3, p < .05 for the smallest difference between two means) revealed that this main effect was due to the difference between the group that received only the auditory sensory input and each of the other groups. There was no difference between the group that received auditory and visual sensory input and the group that received all 3 sources of input. The results were similar when the groups were compared after the 20- minute time delay. Again, the group that received only the auditory sensory input performed the fewest number of correct steps (M = 4.8, S_D = 5.4). After the delay, there was a slightly larger difference between the numbers of correct steps performed by the group that received auditory and visual sensory input (M = 12.4, S_D = 5.8) and the group that received auditory, visual and tactual sensory input (M = 14.1, S_D = 3.5). The F statistic revealed significant group differences (E = 6.7 (df = 2,18), p < 0.007) in the mean number of correct steps performed after the 20-minute time delay. Using the Tukey statistic to test differences among group means (g = 11.3, p < .05 for the smallest differences between means), it was shown that the significant main effect was due to the auditory only group having significantly lower scores than the other two groups. 42 Within Group Effects As was done with the verbal measure, the t-statistic was computed to compare the same group’s non-verbal replication of the task at Times 1 and 2. As shown in Table 3 none of the groups showed a significant difference in the number of correct steps performed before and after the 20-minute delay. The t scores for the auditory-verbal, auditory-visual, and auditory-visual-tactual groups were respectively 0.61, 0.67, 0.09 and all were non-significant even when a liberal alpha level (p > 0.10) was used. 9W Qualitative data were obtained after the completion of both the verbal and non-verbal measures. The participants were asked if there was anything that would have made the task easier to perform. This question was asked in order to gain more insight into how the different sensory channels may have affected the participants’ performances. The outcomes were also expected to help the investigator understand whether any procedural changes could be made to improve the experiment in future studies. When the participants were asked if there was anything that would have made the task easier, the responses varied. Table 4 illustrates the variety of responses obtained from each group. 43 Table 4. Participants’ verbatim responses for making the task easier Group 1 Auditory Input Group 2 Auditory and Visual Input Group 3 Auditory, Visual and Tactual anut ”having [heard the directions] three times complete instead of hearing the directions three times individually" "knowing that the tape would not have accompanied my own attempt” * "nothing would have made it easier” ** “if it had a name indicating the end result" "not going into another room for a distraction” "told in advance I'd be doing the task" ”being able to see someone do it, or if I had the paper in my hand doing it while I listened" ”being able to show how to explain instead of having to tell how to do the task" ”being told how to do it a couple more times” ”hearing the instructions a third time” "maybe seeing it again" "going over the instructions again after the 20 mintue delay” ”having more clear instructions" "being able to listen to the instructions with the paper in hand" "if it wasn't so quiet in here” ”slowing down the last section, or hearing it while I did it" "nothing would have made the task easier” Note. * Indicates that one other participant gave this response Note. ** Indicates that more than one participant gave this response. Participants in the group that received only auditory sensory input tended to give responses that were directed towards the quality of the directions themselves. In only one instance did a participant answer this question by saying that it would have been helpful to receive the instructions (i.e., sensory input) in a different sensory modality than the auditory one. Participants in the group that received auditory and visual sensory input had a variety of responses. However, 2 of the 7 participants stated that the task would have been easier had they been given tactual input. The group that received the auditory, visual and tactual sensory input had the most participants say that nothing would have made the task easier. Two of the 7 participants, however, commented that receiving the instructions a few more times would have been helpful to their performances. In addition to asking the participants if there was anything that would have made the task easier, they were also asked to rate their comfort level, i.e. their level of confidence that the task was done correctly. The scale of 1 to 5 was used; 1 represented least comfortable and 5 the most comfortable. For the group that received auditory sensory input, the rating averaged 2.0. The rating averaged 3.0 for the groups that received auditory-visual sensory input or auditory-visual-tactual sensory input. This latter outcome indicated that the visual and tactual groups felt slightly more confident that the task was done correctly. Finally, each participant was asked whether he judged himself to be an auditory Ieamer or a visual or hands-on Ieamer. The investigator wanted to see if there were any relationships between these reported self-biases in Ieaming and the participants’ actual performances. Table 5 summarizes these results. 45 Table 5. Participants’ remrts of personal Ieaming gpe Judged Learner Type Experimental Group Aud-Vis Aud-Ws-Tac 0 1 Visual 4 4 Hands-On 3 2 Regardless of group assignment, at least half of the participants in each experimental group judged themselves to be visual Ieamers. Across the 3 groups, 13 or 62% of the participants judged themselves as visual Ieamers, whereas 6 or 28% reported themselves to be tactual or hands-on Ieamers. The participants were least inclined to judge themselves as auditory Ieamers (N = 2 or 10%). Therefore it was not surprising that there did not appear to be a direct relationship between the type of Ieamer each participant felt he was and his actual performance in an assigned modality condition. The one participant in the auditory only group who reported himself to be an auditory Ieamer was not able to replicate the nonverbal task. He also was 1 of 6 participants who failed to do so. Just 1 participant in this auditory- verbal group correctly completed the task. He reported himself to be a visual Ieamer. Of the 4 participants in the auditory and visual input group who judged themselves to be visual Ieamers, only one was able to correctly complete the nonverbal task. The other 3 participants in this group reported themselves to be hands-on Ieamers. All of them were able to complete the task. 46 In the auditory-visual-tactual group the two participants who reported themselves as hands-on Ieamers were both able to correctly complete the nonverbal task. It is interesting that 5 or 50% of the participants who did the task correctly reported themselves to be hands-on or tactual Ieamers. The task also was completed by the same number of persons who reported themselves to be visual Ieamers. Conclusion When Ieaming a new procedural task, the sensory input does matter to both the verbal and nonverbal performance as was measured in this study. The results consistently showed that the participants were at a disadvantage for Ieaming a new procedural task when they received just auditory-verbal input, as pilot data had predicted. However, the results did not show uniformly better performance when participants received tactual input in addition to visual input as expected. The group that got the added visual input was just as successful as the added tactual group on some tasks. Since performance seemed enhanced when visual or tactual input was added, it can be assumed then that Ieaming a new procedural task is optimized when other sensory inputs are provided in addition to auditory-verbal input. 47 Chapter Four DISCUSSION The goal of this study was to determine whether performance on a novel task is affected by the sensory modality in which participants receive the instructions for doing an origami task. The participants were 21 male college students. They were randomly assigned to one of three groups that differed in the sensory input condition for receiving task instructions. One group was given the instructions with just auditory verbal input. A second group received visual input in addition to the auditory input. The third group received tactual and visual input in addition to auditory verbal input. Aftenrvards, the participants attempted to do the task and provide a verbal description of how to do the task. The participants’ nonverbal performances were analyzed for the number of correct steps performed before and after the 20-minute time period. Their verbal performances were analyzed for the number of correct information units (ClUs) provided about the task and their nonverbal performances were evaluated in terms of the number of steps performed correctly regardless of whether the origami task was completed. The findings revealed a differential modality effect on the participants’ performances. The data supported some expectations but not others. The experimental group that received just auditory verbal input performed the least well on both the nonverbal and verbal performance measures, as expected. This observation suggests that auditory verbal input alone offered the least information about how to do the kind of novel procedural task used in this study. Performances were significantly better in the groups that received the additional visual or visual and tactile input. However, the expectation that the tactual group would perform better than both the auditory and the auditory-visual groups was only partially supported by the data. The tactual group performed remarkably better on the verbal task but not on the nonverbal task. Even on the verbal task, the tactual group performed better than the other groups only on the second trial, i.e., after the 20-minute interval. The failure to observe consistent significant differences between the groups that received the added visual input and the added visual and tactual inputs was surprising, particularly for the nonverbal task. This outcome for the nonverbal task as well as the outcome of the verbal task may have resulted from some methodological limitations of the study as will be discussed later in this chapter. In sum, the findings were not straightforward. Performances were influenced in a complex way by the (1) presentation modality, i.e., auditory- verbal versus auditory-verbal and visual, versus auditory-verbal, visual and tactile, (2) the timing of performance, i.e., before and after a 20-minute delay for displaying Ieaming (3) the type of performance domain assessed, i.e., verbal versus nonverbal. The following discussion focuses first on interpreting the modality group differences in verbal and nonverbal performances as a function of the memory task. Second, the implications of this study for future research are discussed. The methodological limitations of the current study 49 are discussed within the context of doing future research. Finally some clinical implications of the findings from this study and similar future studies are identified. Integpretation of Verbal Effects This study was guided by the assumption that the different sensory modalities provide different information about verbal events, and these inherent differences may partly explain the outcomes of this study. The Auditogy-Verbal Modality Effect The most consistent outcome of this study was that auditory-verbal input alone was not sufficient to Ieam the novel procedural task well enough to actually do it or talk about how to do it. One reason for this outcome has to do with the serial processing demands of Ieaming an auditory task, as presented in this task. The other reason has to do with the natural limitation of auditory- verbal input alone for initially deriving the semantic content of words from a real event. Serial complexfly of the auditogy-verbal task. By all accounts the auditory verbal task used in this study was very complex. This is because the verbal directions required the sequential processing, memory, and retrieval of so much verbal information in order to understand how to do and talk about the task aftenrvards. For the origami task used, the participants had to hold as many as 17 verbally delivered chunks or sentences in memory before they could Ieam the task well enough to talk about how to do it. Furthermore, the novelty of the task prevented participants from relying too heavily on their 50 previously stored semantic content for the words spoken in the instructions. They were not even given any information about what the final product or task goal was suppose to look or feel like. Given the rapid decay time of auditory input relative to other sensory modalities, the reduced performances observed were not surprising. In contrast to the rapid auditory input with quick decay time the visual modality presents fewer and slower spatio-temporal changes in the perceptual field. The tactual input also involves temporal demands, but its combined serial and spatial properties seem less susceptible to the rapid temporal changes and stimulus decay associated with the auditory modality (Heller 8r Schiff, 1991; Strei 8r Pecheux, 1986). The participants also have more control over how fast or slow to move in getting the tactual input. It is not surprising, therefore, that when questioned about what would have made the task easier, participants in the auditory verbal group more often focused on ways to slow down the input or enhance it in order to decrease the information procession load for getting content into memory. In contrast, just one participant in each of the other experimental groups commented on the need to experience the input more often in order to Ieam it. Role of auditory inp_ut in determining the semantic content pf wp_r_d_s_. Another reason why the auditory—verbal group may have been the least successful in their verbal descriptions of the origami task used in this study is that auditory-verbal input may play a less salient role in getting the Initial semantic content of words from the event context than do other modalities. 51 The auditory word forms are just symbols - they index or represent the objects, actions, relationships, or states that words refer to in the real world; they are not synonymous with them in real world experience. So just hearing the words alone provides no information about what these objects, actions, or states actually are in a real world sense, if they have not already been experienced. In this study, the participants in the auditory-verbal condition received no contextual or event information about the origami task. For example, they did not have direct access to the shape, form, or transformations of object relationships involved in creating the origami product. This view suggests that the auditory-verbal modality is particularly suited to Ieaming the form of the linguistic or verbal input, i.e., the arrangement of phonetic/phonological symbols that conform to the morphosyntactic patterns or rules of a language. But these forms alone do not provide direct input about the meaning or semantic content of the forms in the actual situation. To get the meaning or semantic content of spoken forms, speakers/listeners must relate these heard forms to some events in the real situation or to already stored experiences about linguistic meaning or content. If the retrieval of this stored content is inefficient, as can be the case with traumatic brain injuries, the reception of new content will be hindered. Simply put, a speaker/listener may not comprehend what is being said. Therefore, it is not surprising that the auditory - verbal group performed the least well on both performance measures used in this study. Unless the participants had previous experience with origami tasks, they would be expected to have a difficult time attaching 52 meaning to the spoken words, then it would have been extremely difficult to Ieam how to do the origami task or tell someone else how to do it. Tactual versus Visual Modality Effect As mentioned above, the experimental groups that received task instructions with some visual or some visual and tactual input performed much better than did the auditory-verbal group. When visual or visual and tactual inputs are received along with auditory verbal input, then information is offered in the perceptual stream about the possible semantic content of the auditory/verbal forms used. For example, both visual and tactual inputs provided direct experience with the spatial and temporal aspects of the events involved in doing the origami task. Participants could see the shape and color of the objects used in the event along with the actions used to create the changes that occurred with each folding act. All the same information was available to the participants who got the tactual input. But in addition, they could feel the stimuli and what their bodies had to do to create the actions that resulted in the various steps of doing the origami task. So they could directly experience the cause/effect relationships involved in the act of creating the product from doing the origami task. It should not be surprising then that participants in the experimental groups that got the additional visual or visual and tactual inputs were able to provide more CIU units in the verbal task than were those in the auditory-verbal group. The CIU units were based on semantic content. However, the prediction that the group with added tactual and visual input would do better than the group with just added visual input was not confirmed for both verbal tasks. It has been reasoned that receiving 3 sources of input about an event as in the case of the tactual group would provide more information for Ieaming the origami task than two sources as in the case of the visual group. While the differences between these two groups were not significant before the 20-minute break, they were significant in the expected direction after the 20-minute waiting period. The participants who got the added tactual experience performed better than those who got just the added visual experience for doing the origami task. This outcome, though not expected, was similar to the results reported by Stockman and Latham (2001) for typically developing preschoolers who participated in a novel juice making task. The results of the present study are important because they showed a trend toward a tactual input bias even for mature, normally functioning adults. This observation, though preliminary, is important. This is because of the prevailing tendency to view the “hands-on” Ieaming as essentially relevant to young child Ieamers and not to mature Ieamers. This study suggests, however, that the tactual input may enhance adult Ieaming of novel procedural tasks. This was the case despite the assumption that the college males used in this study had abundant auditory and visual processing experiences from attending class lectures. 54 The connectionist model proposed by Rumelhart and McClelland (1986) supports why this kind of result was seen. The main idea of this model is that information is stored in multiple locations throughout the brain. These locations form a network of connections. The more connections one has to new information, the more likely it is to be remembered. By providing the participants with the tactual input, they were provided with additional connections to the new information that the participants in the other experimental groups did not receive. Therefore, it can be assumed that based upon the connectionist model, the participants that received tactual input would have a greater chance of remembering the newly Ieamed information. A modality bias favoring the tactual modality may not have been seen until after the 20-minute time period because of the complexity of the task. The participants had to process information from multiple sensory modalities. Moreover, they had to do so quickly and in a serial manner. This is because the sensory inputs were not simultaneously presented. They first heard the verbal instruction. Then they had to watch the examiner to at least begin an action step before trying to do the same step on their own. This meant that instead of receiving all of the sensory input at once, the participants had to coordinate the inputs one at a time as they received them. They had to quickly switch back and forth between watching the examiner doing the task and then actually doing the task themselves. This forced the participants to remember what they had just previously heard, saw or done. Remembering these temporal events could have increased memory load and strained performances in similar ways to the processing of auditory input. This integration of multiple input sources may have in fact impeded participants’ performances instead of enhancing them at first. The results of this study with adults are consistent with the observations that children’s performances can be degraded when multimodal information must be processed. For example, Stockman and Latham (2001) reported that preschoolers who received tactual input during a novel juice making task had significantly better verbal performances than did those who received visual and auditory input from a video of the juice making event. However, the tactual group performed better only on their second trial - not the first one. Gazella (1997) and Gazella and Stockman (2003) suggest that even the combined auditory and visual input in a story presentation and retelling task may tax processing resources more than when a story is presented in just the auditory-verbal modality. Gillam, Cowan and Marler (1998) reported that children with specific language impairment have more difficulty using phonological features of auditory verbal codes during tasks requiring “ multiple mental operations” such as speaking and pointing at the same time. Bloom (1996) proposed the cognitive cost hypothesis to account for the observed decrease in verbal performance when children engage in doing more than one thing at the same time. In one study, Bloom and her colleagues observed whether children expressed emotion and talk at the same time. They looked at children whose first words were emerging and children who were going through the vocabulary spurt. Children who were just beginning to 56 talk either talked or expressed emotion, but they did not do both at the same time. At older ages, children were able to talk and express emotion at the same time. Yet the words used tended to be both familiar and routine. Bloom and her colleagues (1996) also observed how children talked during play. She found that when the children played with unfamiliar toys, their talk was limited. These children also demonstrated limited emotional affect when playing with unfamiliar toys. Tomasello and Kruger (1995) did a study in which 2;0 year old children were taught a new word before, during, or after an event. They found that children were less likely to Ieam a novel word when it was presented at the same time as the event. This outcome was interpreted in terms of processing constraints. Tomasello and Kruger suggested that hearing the new word during the event required the children to process double the information (i.e., information about the word and about the event). This increased the task demands for these children. In the present study, however, the initial linkages between auditory- verbal forms and their visual semantic content were apparently not strong enough to show Ieaming in both verbal trials. Othenrvise, this same group would have improved their verbal performances in the second trial just like those in the tactual group. But since this did not happen, one can infer that the added visual support was less effective than added visual and tactual support for Ieaming the task used in this study. This temporal effect on displaying a 57 modality difference is a very interesting and unexpected finding from this study. Obviously more research is needed as discussed next. lntegpretatlon of Nonverbal Effects Auditogy Verbal Modality Effects The nonverbal performances of the individuals who just received the auditory-verbal input were not at all surprising. It was expected that of the three experimental groups, these individuals would perform the least number of correct steps to complete the task, given the pilot study results. It was not expected that the words heard by participants would be enough to Ieam to do the task unless there was already some prior experience with origami as discussed already. The results supported that expectation. In fact, during the Ieaming phase of the experiment, some individuals in the auditory only group appeared restless in their seats or sighed loudly - behaviors that can be interpreted as a sign of feeling overwhelmed. All but one participant in this group gave up when trying to do the task nonverbally. This outcome suggests that the auditory-verbal input alone offers limited benefit for Ieaming a novel procedural task well enough to perform it or talk about it. Nonetheless, the fact that one participant was able to complete the task shows that it was not an impossible task to Ieam. However it is Interesting that this participant pantomimed the steps as he heard them presented auditorally. That is, he was seen working through the steps with his hands without the paper while listening to the instructions. Such an observation supports the importance of getting input from more than the auditory modality 58 Visual and Tactual Modality Effects What proved to be a surprising result is that there appeared to be no performance advantage for the experimental group that received the additional tactual and visual input over the group that received just the additional visual input. Just one more participant (5 or 71%) in the group with tactual input performed the nonverbal task compared to the 4 or 57% in the group that received just the added visual input. Furthermore, there were no significant differences between the visual and tactual group in the number of steps completed for doing the task on neither the first nor the second trial. The absence of a modality difference here suggests that the added tactual input was less vital to Ieaming how to perform the novel procedural task nonverbally for these adult Ieamers than it was for the verbal task. In fact, the number of steps completed for the nonverbal task hardly shifted from time 1 (before the 20-minute time interval) to time 2 (after the 20-minute time interval) for either group. No more than half of the participants actually did the nonverbal task. 80 many participants were not able to do the task regardless of whether they received instructions in one or more modalities. It is predicted that participants needed to have more input trials than were given in this study. This is not surprising. Learning what to do would have required a high level of cross-modal integration by both groups, i.e., auditory and visual integration in one group and auditory, visual and tactual integration in the other group. The research on children, as described above, suggest that there are performance <( research on children, as described above, suggest that there are performance costs when Ieamers have to process more than one source of information at the same time. However, the information processing demands must not have been the same for the verbal and nonverbal tasks used in this study. Although, the tactual group did not increase its verbal performance in the first trial, its verbal performance improved significantly in the second trial. The same second trial effect was not observed for their nonverbal performances. The differences between the verbal and nonverbal performances of the tactual group may be related to biases in the way the experiment was setup in addition to any factors related to processing demands on performance. On both the first and second trials, the participants in this study always performed the nonverbal task before the verbal task. Therefore the verbal performances could always have been aided by the prior nonverbal performances, but nonverbal performances could never profit from having verbally described the steps for doing the task before hand. Failure to randomly test verbal and nonverbal performances created a confound in the data. So it is unknown how much of the verbal advantage observed for the tactual group on trial 2 was due to the consolidation of memory for the task during the 20-minute time gap or to access to prior experience with doing the task (or at least attempting to do the task) before a verbal description was given. Nevertheless, the question remains as to why performance was not elevated in the first or second trials for the participants in the other two experimental groups, namely those in the 60 auditory-verbal group and those in the auditory-verbal and visual groups. Their verbal descriptions also had the opportunity to benefit from these participants’ prior attempts to do the action. Yet their verbal descriptions did not improve on Trial 2 like those in the tactual group. Cleariy more research is needed, as discussed next. Implications for Further Research This study provides interesting information to support the claim that tactual sensory input is important even when adults are Ieaming new procedural tasks. But further research in this area is necessary to gain a better understanding of the tactual modality’s role in Ieaming such tasks. Although motivated by the clinical intervention needs of persons with traumatic brain injury, this study did not focus on a clinical population. So research obviously is needed about whether the performance trends of this study apply to a clinical population with TBI. At the same time, research needs to continue focusing on the varying roles that different sensory systems may play in Ieaming different types of tasks even among normal Ieamers. Several data trends in this study supported the expectation that the tactual input is helpful to Ieaming how to do a procedural task even for smart normal adult Ieamers, but more rigorous research on larger numbers of participants is needed to defend any claims about the role of the tactual input in Ieaming novel procedural tasks. The following discussion singles out several factors that may have contributed to the failure to reveal a stronger effect in this study. They relate to 61 methodological and research design limitations that should be taken into account in designing future studies. Methodolpglcal Limitations In the Present Study There are several reasons why the results of this study may not have confirmed the predictions or expectations for the nonverbal task. Order of scoring the time 1 and time 2 data. First, there may have been a methodological flaw in how the judges scored the verbal data. All performances before the time delay were scored before the performances after the time delay. Failure to randomize the order in which the two trials were judged may have confounded the outcomes. The tactual group may have performed better in the second trial than the first trial because the judges had become more familiar with the scoring procedures by the second trial. But this interpretation must be tempered by the fact that the performances for the other groups did not increase significantly from time 1 to time 2. Still a post-hoc analysis was done to determine whether such a confounding practice effect might have occurred. This could be done because the judges had scored just a third of the data for the reliability analysis, which was described in Chapter 2. Altogether, each judge scored the performances of 4 new participants such that Trial 1 performances were scored first for two participants while Trial 2 performances were scored first for others in a random assignment. The 12 new scores were calculated and pooled across the examiners and added to the previously scored data for a new statistical analysis. The F test revealed no significant differences among the 3 experimental groups’ verbal 62 performances before the time delay (E (2,18) = 0.46 , p = 0.64). This outcome was consistent with those already described in the results chapter. Results of the F test after the time delay also revealed no significant trend (E (2.18) = 2.12 , p = .15). This latter outcome contrasts with the significant group differences already described in chapter 3, which showed a significant tactual effect. Although this post-hoc analysis revealed no significant difference, the tactual group still had a larger mean number of verbal ClUs than did the other groups. The means and standard deviations were 118.3 (_S_Q = 19.06), 87.4 ($541.4) and 80.6 (E =43.7) for the tactual, auditory-verbal and visual groups, respectively. A series of two sample t tests revealed that only the difference between the tactual and visual groups in the post hoc analysis ( = - 2.09, p < .07) reflected a significant trend. But no significant differences were observed between the auditory-verbal and the tactual group ( t = 1.79, p =11) nor between the auditory-verbal and the visual group ( t= .43, p = .77). So apparently, the tactual group still retained an edge in its performances over the other groups after the time delay even in this post hoc comparison with randomized data sets. But this potential effect needs to be tested more rigorously in future research. When taken together, the results of this study suggest that the order in which the data before and after the time delay were scored may have affected the results. Another study in which the verbal performances are randomized during scoring is necessary to determine if the results would differ from those obtained in this study. 63 Number of participants. The expected results might not have been obtained simply because too few participants were included in the study. Just 7 participants were included in each experimental setting. The 21 participants is a fairly small number, making it difficult to get a real We picture of central tendencies. Future research studies should include a greater number of participants. Tyg of pppulation. A third reason why a stronger expected effect was not observed may have been due to the population studied. This study used young college age adults with normal developmental histories. As college students, these participants are likely to have very high levels of experience with processing auditory-verbal and visual information, just to get lecture content in college classes. So the differential effects of sensory input may be less obvious for a novel task than they might be in other populations with less of this kind of experience. Besides studying adult clinical populations with impaired brain function like TBI, research should include young typically and atypically developing children as well. Future studies can be designed to test whether a tactual input bias is more salient for younger than older Ieamers. Providing the tactual input in the Ieaming condition. The way in which the task was done could be another reason why the expected results were not obtained. It should be noted that a valid clinical application of guided interaction clinical practices was not done in this study. A valid application of guided interaction requires the clinician to guide the patients hands and body 64 to do a task so that tactual input from causal actions are experienced simultaneously with their auditory and visual effects as in real life events. However this investigator had not been trained to do guided interaction intervention. So it was not done in this study. But future studies can be done to determine if the manner of delivering the tactual input is indeed a variable in Ieaming a new task. Perhaps the expected verbal results were seen only after the time delay because participants needed the extra time to consolidate so much new information, given the complexity of the task. Because this task had a serial component it is reasonable to assume that this information took longer for the participants to process. A more accurate representation of the effects of tactual sensory input may have been seen had the participants received the tactual input through guided interaction. The guided interaction would have decreased the complexity of the task by eliminating the serial nature of the sensory input integration. By eliminating the serial nature of the task, there would have been less stress on the participants’ memory. Craik and Lockhart (1972) proposed the levels-of-processing theory. It might explain why the expected verbal results were seen after the time delay. This theory of information processing claims that when Ieaming a new task, we use different levels of elaboration as information is received. People process information first by attending to or orienting to the stimuli. Then they label the new information, and finally, they give meaning to the new lnforrnation. The main point of this theory however, is that any of the stimuli 65 acted upon by sensory receptor cells will be permanently stored in memory. But the level at which the information was processed will determine how the information is retrieved from memory. While the expected results were not seen for the nonverbal task in this study, it does not mean that the same outcome would hold for other populations. Type of procedural task used. Clearly task complexity interacts with the type of stimuli with which Ieamers interact. Thus a final reason why the expected results for the nonverbal performance measure were not observed may have been because of the task itself. This study was a paper task. The paper may not have been heavy enough to give the amount of tactual input needed to provide adequate information. Also, this task had a lot of steps. Perhaps a task with fewer steps may have elicited the expected results. Theoretical Issues There also are theoretical reasons to continue the investigative focus on the roles of different sensory modalities in Ieaming, particularly as they may relate to our understanding of how memory works. Given the bias in both science and clinical practices toward Ieaming and remembering auditory and visual stimuli, it is not surprising that less is known about the tactual system by comparison. On the other hand, the role that auditory memory deficits seem to play in the Ieaming of language among children with specific language impairment now is well documented (see for example, Gillam, Cowan, 8r Marler, 1998; Marler, Champlin, & Gillam, 2001) among others. 66 However, the present study hinted that the memory processes associated with tactual Ieaming may differ from those involved in auditory verbal and visual Ieaming, and that multimodal Ieaming may pose different processing demands on Ieaming and remembering than does the Ieaming from a single modality. Intuitively, the organization of tactual sources of input seems to be at the core of procedural Ieaming and memory. This is because procedural Ieaming involves action sequences. The data trends in this study point to the possibility that tactual memory processes differ from the more frequently studied auditory-verbal memory processes. But we are likely to know less about this kind of Ieaming and memory compared to auditory verbal per se. Glenberg (1997) is among those who make the case for conceptualizing memory systems in terms of action patterns. He reported that memory for actions (performing a command such as “open the book”) is better than memory for the verbal descriptions of the commands.” (Glenberg, 1997, p.5). Future research can determine to what extent this kind of finding holds for different test conditions and populations of Ieamers. Clinical Implications This study was motivated by the goal of finding out about how the delivery of clinical services to individuals with a TBI may possibly be improved. The focus on the varying effects of different sensory input modalities on Ieaming a procedural task were studied because of biases in the way speech and language therapy is conducted. Typically treatment sessions apply auditory and/or visual inputs to Ieaming a new skill. Less often, do we see 67 auditory and/or visual inputs to Ieaming a new skill. Less often, do we see tactual input used in a clinical setting. Yet the strongest and most consistent result of this study was that auditory verbal input alone resulted in the poorest performance for both the verbal semantic and nonverbal performances. The results of this study coupled with further research may show some benefit to incorporating tactual input into treatment sessions at least for some types of tasks. This study used a procedural task, which is very much like the way in which individuals Ieam new tasks in their natural settings. Therefore, it should be considered that when working with clients in a therapeutic setting other sensory systems might be of benefit, auditory-verbal and visual input sources may not be the only helpful sources of input for stimulating the Ieaming of new tasks. Severe patients, in particular, often need additional support. Tactual input may provide the necessary foundation from which these individuals can begin to re-establish their damaged skills. If future research bears out the assumption that tactual input enhances Ieaming, then the way treatment is done routinely would have to change. There should be less emphasis on the paper/pencil type tests especially for severe patients than on functional tasks that are likely to combine sensory inputs in ways that include the tactual sources of input associated with action/interaction in the environment. Fortunately, there are signs that therapy models are moving in the direction of creating more functional environments for retooling and retraining persons with acquired brain damage including 652 TBI patients are now being placed in community-based intervention programs (M. Sadasivan, personal communication, April 18, 2003). This type of treatment requires the individual to perform functional tasks in an actual real-world situation. Treatment tasks are not artificial in nature. At the same time the patient is provided with multi-modal input that includes the tactual modality. As suggested by this study, multi-modal input may provide the necessary input to establish memories for new Ieaming, and access to those skills already Ieamed or affected by the injury. This kind of trend in service delivery is supported by the trends of this study. They suggest that multimodal sources of input are likely to be helpful to reestablishing the linkages between auditory verbal forms and their semantic content. The tactual input may turn out to be especially critical to give in the multimodal stream of input for severe patients. If this type of benefit can be seen with normal adults, whose previous experiences have not been disrupted or clinically challenged, then it is likely that the same type of benefit will be observed for individuals whose ability to connect stored experience with auditory verbal forms in actual situations has become impaired. In fact, the benefit might possibly be even greater for TBI individuals as was the case in the Latham and Stockman (2001) study of children with special needs. They reported that when Ieaming a novel procedural task, children with language Ieaming difficulties showed even more dramatic changes in verbal performances than did typically developing children when tactual input was given as opposed to just visual and auditory input. 69 This study suggests that tactual sensory information may play an important role in Ieaming procedural tasks even for adult Ieamers without cognitive disability. Other studies too, such as Stockman and Latham's (2001) work with normal developing preschoolers support the importance of tactual sensory input in Ieaming new tasks whereas the present study suggests that tactual input may play an important role in normal adult Ieaming of novel procedural tasks. As more supporting empirical support for this effect become available, the orientation to speech and language therapy will need to be modified. Clinicians will need to consider the “doing” of a task as sensory input instead of the conventional thinking of the “doing” as only performance output. Conclusion In conclusion, the results of this study suggest that tactual sensory input may well provide valuable information for normal adults Ieaming a novel procedural task under some conditions. This study suggests that receiving auditory-verbal input alone is not sufficient for Ieaming a novel procedural task. Further research needs to be conducted to determine how these results generalize to different populations and tasks. But information from this study potentially can stimulate more research and theorizing about the role of tactile input in the delivery of clinical services to both children and adults with linguistic and cognitive impairments. 7O APPENDICES 71 APPENDIX A Participant Characteristics Table 1. Participant Characteristics Participant # Major Age 1 engineering 18 2 hospitality business 18 3 advertising 20 4 telecommunications 21 5 hospitality business 20 6 turf grass management 20 7 communications 21 8 advertising 20 9 journalism 18 10 communications 21 11 hospitality business 20 12 hospitality business 20 13 general management 19 14 undeclared 18 1 5 psychology 21 16 communications 21 17 building construction 20 management 72 18 finance 1 9 19 pre-medical 19 20 international relations 18 21 undeclared 19 Mean Age = 19.5 73 APPENDIX B Participant Recruitment Flier Students are being sought to participate in a research study to be conducted in the Communication Arts and Sciences Building. Subjects must be: 1. Male 2. Caucasian 3. Between the ages of 18 and 21 All participants will be reimbursed for their time at a rate of $12.00/hour. Janelle Rowe, a master’s student in speech-language pathology, is conducting this study. This study will be looking at the area of new Ieaming. Dr. Ida Stockman will be overseeing the study. If you would like to participate in this study, please contact: Janelle Rowe rowejane@msu.edu (989) 686-3165 V... APPENDIX C Participant Recruitment Flier for COM 100 Students Folding for Dollars Or Credit Location: Room 14 Basement of COMM ARTS . This study will be taking a closer look at how new Ieaming occurs. Participants must be: male, Caucasian, between the ages of 18-21, communication majors (preferred, but not required). Name of Researcher: Janelle Rowe Researcher’s phone: (989) 686- 3165 Researcher’s email: rowejane@msu.edu Number of Credit Hours (if chosen): 1.5 Monetary reimbursement (if chosen): $12 Appendix D Participant Background lnfonnation Background Information Name: Age: Sex: M F Student: Y N Major: Race/Ethnicity: 1. Is your hearing within normal limits, or corrected to within normal limits? 2. Is your vision within normal limits, or corrected to within normal limits? 3. Do you have a history of brain injury? 4. Do you have a history of Ieaming disability? 5. Do you have experience with origami? Subject Number: OFFICE OF RESEARCH ETHICS AND STANDARDS University Committee on lee-arch Involving than Subjects Michigli Slate University 202 Old: Hall East Using, Ml 48824 517/356-2180 FAX: 517/432-4503 Web; mm Muse/wins EM: ucrihsansucdu Mammal: (firmly omcwuramiowm- Wow twisanxfirrrm-xtm Ind-W nstndm APPENDIX E Approval to Use Human Subjects MICHIGAN STATE UNIVERSITY May 1, 2002 TO: Ida STOCKMAN 378 Com. Arts and Sciences RE: IRE: 02.283 CATEGORY: EXPEDITED 2-4, 2-6 APPROVAL DATE: April 24, 2002 TITLE: SOLVING OF A NOVEL TASK UNDER DIFFERENT MODALITY INPUT CONDITIONS ' The University Committee on Research Involving Human Subjects’ (UCRIHS) review of this project is complete and I am pleased to advise that the rights and welfare of the human subjects appear to be adequately protected and methods to obtain informed consent are appropriate. Therefore. the UCRIHS approved this project. Staff Note: The stamped consent has a small grammatical change in section 7b. Be sure to revise your "electronic” version accordingly. RENEWALS: UCRIHS approval is valid for one calendar year, beginning with the approval data shown above. Projects continuing beyOnd one year must be renewed with the green renewal form. A maximum of four such expedited renewals possible. Investigators wishing to continue a project beyond that time need to submit it again for a complete review. . REVISIONS: UCRIHS must review. any changes in procedures involving human subjects, prior to initiation of the change. If this is done at the time of renewal, please use the green renewal form. To revise an approved protocol at any other time during the year, send your written request to the UCRIHS Chair, requesting revised approval and referencing the project’s lRBt and title. Include in your request a description of the change and any revised instruments, consent forms or advertisements that are applicable. PROBLEMSICHANGES: Should either of the following arise during the course of the work, notify UCRIHS promptly: 1) problems (unexpected side effects, complaints, etc.) involving human subjects or 2) changes in the research environment or new information indicating greater risk to the human subjects than existed when the protocol was previously reviewed and approved. "' If we can be of further assistance, please contact us at (517) 355-2180 or via email: UCRIHSQmsuedu. Please note that all UCRIHS forms are located on the web: http:/Mwmsueduluser/ucrihs Sincerely / (JAM/w Ashir Ku ar, M.D. UCRIHS Chair AK: br 00? Janelle M. Rowe 8200 Kensington Blvd. #719 Davison, Ml 48423 7"] APPENDIX F CONSENT FORM FOR HUMAN SUBJECTS PARTICIPATING IN AN INVESTIGATION Title of Investigation: Completing a Novel Task Under Different Sensory Input Modality Conditions Investigator: Janelle Rowe, MA (c) Department: Graduate student in speech-language pathology at Michigan State University Address: 508 W. Ohio St. Bay City, MI 48706 1. Purpose: a. You are being asked to participate as a research subject in a study of new Ieaming. b. You have been selected as a possible subject in this study because you are male, Caucasian, and between the ages of 18 and 21. c. In the entire study, 24 people are being asked to participate. d. From this study the investigator hopes to Ieam how new Ieaming occurs. e. Your participation in this study will take about one hour. 2. Potential Benefits: a. The potential benefit to you for taking part in this study is an increased understanding of how you Ieam. 3. Potential Risks: a. There are no foreseeable risks associated with this study. 4. What the investigator will do: a. The investigator will conduct an interview with you to ensure your eligibility for participation in the study. Once eligibility is determined, the investigator will teach you a new task. 5. What you will do: 3. Your role in the study is to merely listen to the investigator and follow her instructions. 6. Confidentiality: a. The investigator will keep the information obtained in this study confidential within the limits of the law. Any information obtained from this study that can be identified with you will remain confidential and will not be given to anyone without your permission. The results of this study may be published or presented at professional meetings, but the identities of the research subjects will remain anonymous. You may obtain the results of this research by contacting the investigator. 7. Your Rights: The right to say no and to withdraw: a. Your participation in this research project is completely voluntary. You have the right to refuse. You do not have to be a subject in this study. If you refuse, you will not be contacted again. If you agree, you may change your mind and say no at any time and withdraw from the study. 6. If you have questions or concerns regarding yours or participants’ rights in this study, or are dissatisfied at any time with any aspect of this research, you may contact — anonymously, if you wish - Ashir Kumar, M.D., Chair of the University Committee of Research Involving Human Subjects (UCRIHS) by phone: (517) 355-2180, fax: (517)432-4503, e- mail: pcrihs@msu_.edu_, or regular mail: 202 Olds Hall, East Lansing, MI 48824. 8. Costs and payment for being in the study: 3. There is no cost for being a part of this study. All subjects will be reimbursed for their time at a rate of $12.00/hour. b. If chosen, participants may receive class credit for their participation. 9. The right to get more information: a. If you have any questions, please ask the investigator. The investigators phone number is 989-686-3165. You will be given a copy of this form to keep. 10. Decision and Signatures: YOU ARE MAKING A DECISION WHETHER OR NOT TO PARTICIPATE IN THIS STUDY. YOUR SIGNATURE MEANS THAT YOU HAVE READ THIS CONSENT AND THAT YOU HAVE AGREED TO BE A SUBJECT. Date Time (AM/PM) Signature Signature of Witness Signature of Investigator 9. Appendix G Steps to Origami Project . Position the paper vertically in front of you. Fold the paper in half, bringing the top to the bottom and crease. Take the top flap and fold the paper up to the crease and crease the fold. Unfold Turn the paper over. Take the top flap and fold the paper up to the crease and crease the fold. Unfold Take the bottom left corner and fold it up to the crease in the middle of the page. Repeat the previous step with the remaining three comers. 10.Fold the top flap along the middle crease. 1 1 .Tum the paper over. 12.Fold the top flap along the middle crease. 13.Take the top flap and fold it up, creating a rectangle with a flat surface. 14.Tum the paper vertically. 15.Make a one-inch fold at the top of the paper, creating two pockets. 16.Insert the bottom comers into the pockets. 17.Fold the paper in half, bringing the left side over to the right. APPENDIX H Pre-experiment Demonstration of Vocabulary Define the Following: 1 . Vertical 2. Crease 3. Flap 4. Rectangle APPENDIX I Auditory Group Instructions Group One- Task Instructions Today I will be teaching you a task. Listen to the instructions on the audiotape. After all of the instructions, you will perform the task. APPENDIX K Auditory, Visual, and Tactual Group Instructions Group Three- Task Instructions Today I am going to teach you a task. Listen to the instructions on the audiotape. After each instruction, do the step along with me. After all the instructions you will perform the task. Appendix L Data Collection Sheet Subject Number: Group: Auditory-Verbal Auditory-VerbaWisual Auditory-VerbalNisual/Tactile Nonverbal Performance Measure: Was subject_able to complete the task? Yes No How many trials did it take to complete? 1 2 3 4 5 Please describe how you would instruct someone to do this task: Comfort level with the task (1 being least, and 5 being most): 1 2 3 4 5 Is there something that would have made the task easier? Subject Number". 86 APPENDIX M CORRECT INFORMATION UNIT CALCULATION GUIDELINES (Taken from Nicholas, LE, 8 Brookshire, RH. (1993). A system for quantifying the inforrnativeness and efficiency of the connected speech of adults with aphasia. go_umal of Speech and Hearing Research. 36. 338-350.) Rules for scoring and counting words and correct information units (ClUs) Prior to determining which words should be included in counts of words and correct information units, delete statements that are made before or after the speaker performs the task or suggests that the speaker is ready to begin or has finished the task and do not provide information about the picture(s) or topic itself. Such statements generally are not produced consistently by speakers from one session to another and are deleted to help stabilized counts across sessions. 0 I hope I can remember how I did this before. I’ll start by saying this. I’m supposed to tell you about washing dishes. I’m ready to start. That’s about it. I can’t say any more. The end. 0 That’s about what our Sundays are like. These statements should be grammatically separate from discussion of the picture(s) or topic. The following first statements by a speaker would be included in the word count. 0 In the first picture, the man is angry. 0 Well first of all, there’s a couple fighting. 0 Okay, there’s a man and a woman. 0 Well now, here’s a picture of a party. This does not include commentary on the task or on the speaker’s performance that occurs m the speaker is discussing the picture(s) or topic. (See 1.22 for rules about commentary.) Instructions: Draw a horizontal line through the middle of words that are to be deleted prior to making decisions about the word count. 1.0. COUNTING WORDS Definition: To be included in the word count, words must be intelligible in context to someone who knows the picture(s) or topic being discussed. Context refers to what the scorer knows about the picture(s) or topic and what the scorer knows from the speaker‘s prior words. Words do npt have to be 87 accurate, relevant, or informative relative to the picture(s) or topic being discussed to be included in the word count. Instructions: Cross out with red Xs words that are not to be included in the word count. RULES FOR COUNTING WORDS 1.1. DO NOT COUNT THEOLLOWING 1.11. Words or partial words that are not intelligible in context to someone who knows the picture(s) or topic being discussed. 0 He went to the frampi. c That appears to be a norble. - He had a st...sn...steak. 1.12. Nonword filler (um, er, uh). (See 1.23 and 1.24 for a rule dealing with filler words and phrases, interjection, and informal terms.) 1.2. QOUNT THE FOLLOWING 1.21. All words that are intelligible in context. Count words that contain sound substitutions, omissions, distortions, or additions if the word is intelligible in context (hiscup for hiccup). If the incorrect production results in another real word that does not appear to be the target word, it is still included in the word count (paper for pepper). 1.22. Commentary on the task, on the speaker’s performance, or on the speaker's experiences. 0 This is pretty hard. 0 I can’t think of that word. a No, that’s not right. 1.23. Filler words and phrases (you know, I mean, okay). Do not count nonword filler. (See 1.12) 1.24. lnterjections (oh, oh boy, wow, golly, gosh, gee, aha, hmm) and informal terms (uh-huh [affirmative], un-uh [negative], nope, yep, yeah). 1.25. Common contractions or simplifications of words (gonna for going to, sorta for sort of, em for them). Contractions (both standard [don’t, he’s] and colloquial [gonna, sortal) are counted as two words. 1.26. Each word in hyphenated words (jack-in-the-box = 4 words). 1.27. Each word in numbers (twenty-two = 2 words, one hundred thirty- four = 4 words, nineteen fifty-five = 3 words). 1.28. Compound words as one word (pancake, cowboy). 1.29. Each word in proper names (Mary Smith, St. Paul, Mason City = 2 words each). 1.30. Count acronyms as one word (VA, VFW, TWA = 1 word each). 2.0. COUNTING CORRECT INFORMATION UNITS (ClUs) Definition: Correct information units are words that are intelligible in context, accurate in relation to the picture(s) or topic, and relevant to and informative about the content of the picture(s) or the topic. Words do _n_o_t have to be used in a grammatically correct manner to be included in the correct lnfonnation count. Each correct lnfonnation unit consists of a single word and only words that have been included in the word count can be considered for inclusion in the correct information unit count. Instructions: Put a diagonal penciled slash through words that are pgt to be included in the correct information count. RULES FOR COUNTING ClUs 2.1. DO NOT COUNT THE FOLLOWING (In this section, words in bold print would po_t be counted as correct information units.) 2.11. Words that do not accurately portray what is in the picture(s) or that do not seem accurate in relation to the topic being discussed, such as incorrect names, pronouns, numbers, actions, etc. If a word reflects regional usage (such as calling the midday meal “dinner" in some areas), it is counted as a correct information unit. If grammatical incorrectness would lead to misunderstanding or uncertainty about the meaning of words, the grammatically incorrect words would not be counted as correct lnfonnation units. (See 3.12 for examples of grammatically incorrect words that would be counted as correct information units). a The girl is riding her bike. (The picture shows a girl with a bike nearby which she may have been riding, but which she is not currently riding.) a The girl is on a ladder. She fell. (The picture shows a boy on a stool who is tipping, but has not fallen yet.) 0 The boys and girls are arriving. (The picture shows only one boy and one girl arriving.) If several people are involved in an action and only one of them is mentions, the mentioned one is still counted as a correct information unit. This constitutes an incomplete description but not an inaccurate one. The fly is arriving. (The picture shows a boy and a girl arriving.) The _m_a_n_ drove away. (The picture shows a couple driving away.) 2.12. Attempts to correct sound errors in words except for the final attempt. c He put paper popper pepper on his food. 0 She saw her with her mass...mack. . .mask. 2.13. Dead ends, false starts, or revisions in which the speaker begins an utterance but either revises it or leaves it uncompleted and uninfonnative with regard to the picture(s) or topic. My si...no no not my sister...my fa...with my wife. He goes over to her and puts his wants to give her a hug. He looks out and sees that she had the car ran into the tree. In the nose in the mouse in the house. We go to a party no I mean a movie. If an utterance is incomplete, but some information about the picture(s) or topic has been given, count that information. o The kitchen window was... In this example, the words _t_he k_itchen was would be counted as correct information units (if they meet the other criteria). Even through the entire statement was not completed, the words are informative. Words that express some legitimate uncertainty or change in perception about characters, events, or settings in a picture are counted as correct information units (if they meet the other criteria). See 2.18 for further examples. 0 Her (_t_aLd or maybe a neighbor was in the tree. a From the looks of the candles, he must be fpgr. No there is a_nother candle on the table sp he must be five years old. 2.14 Repetition of words or ideas that do not add new information to the utterance, are not necessary for cohesion or grammatical correctness, and are not purposely used to intensify meaning. 0 The blue truck was blue. 0 The restaurant was a new one. It was a new restaurant. 0 She was cleaning washing the dishes. Such repetition of words or ideas can be separated by other counted words. 0 The mother was very angry. The daughter was crying. The mother was very mad. Exceptions: (a) If the repeated words or ideas are necessary for cohesion, they are counted. She went to the store. The §t0_r_e_ was closed. (b) If words are repeated to achieve effect or to intensify a statement they are counted. 0 The girl was vegy, vegy sad. 0 They were fighting, really fighting. (c) If repeated words are used to expand on previous information, they are counted. 0 He put on a fine left shoe. 0 There were some people...a mpg and a woman. 2.15. The first use of a pronoun for which an unambiguous referent has not been provided. Subsequent uses of the pronoun for the same unspecified or ambiguous referent are counted as correct information units ( if they meet the other criteria). 0 She (no referent) was doing the dishes. I think _S_h§ was day- dreaming. If an inaccurate referent is provided but it is clear that a pronoun refers back to it, the pronoun would be counted as a correct information unit. a The fox (inaccurate referent) ate some of the cake and i_t was hiding. 2.16. Vague or nonspecific words or phrases that are not necessary for the grammatical completeness of a statement and for which the subject has not provided a more specific word or phrase. The mother is drying one of those things. She gave him some stuff. He put something up to the tree but that one knocked it down. We had pancakes or scrambled eggs or something like that. Of o I wash the glasses and plates and so on. The words ”here” and “there” frequently fall into this category. Here we have a boy. This here boy is crying. That mother there is doing dishes. There is a eat here and a dog there. The mother is there. She put them over here. She has a bike there. The cookies were up there. The following are examples of uses of “here” and “there” that are necessary for the grammatical completeness of the statement and cannot be replaced by a more specific word. These uses of “here” and “there” would be counted as correct lnfonnation units. 0 There is a boy. 0 El_e_rp comes the same couple. The following is an example of a nonspecific word that is preceded by a clear referent and would be counted as a correct information unit. 0 The boy opened the cupboard. The cookies were up Mp. 2.17. Conjunctive terms (particularly so and then) if they are used indiscriminately as filler or continuants rather than as cohesive ties to connect ideas. 0 There is a man. Then there is a woman and then a cat. When used cohesively, “then” indicates the temporal order or sequential organization of things or events. 0 She had lunch and Mpg she went to the store. 0 When you go into my house you see the living room first, they) the dining room, then the kitchen. When used cohesively, “so” indicates a casual consequence. a He was thirsty §p he drank some juice. a The mother was after the dog g2 the boy was crying. 2.18. Qualifiers and modifiers if they are used indiscriminately as filler or are used unnecessarily in descriptions of events, settings, or characters that are unambiguously pictured. The following examples concern unambiguously pictured information. Apparently this is a kitchen. Evidently the boy is on a stool. I think that the cat is in the tree. It looks like the man is up in the tree too. The boy is sort of crying and the dog is kind of hiding. 0 Of course, the woman left in a huff. When used infonnatively, qualifiers and modifiers suggest legitimate uncertainty on the part of the speaker about events, settings, or characters portrayed in the picture(s) or modify associated words in a meaningful way. The following examples concern ambiguously pictured information. Apparently this is a mother and her two children. I think she is his sister. It looks like he gave them the wrong directions. She must be daydreaming. He might be the girl’s dad or mame he’s a neighbor. He is the father or a neighbor. I don’t know which. He looks sort of sad. 0 Evidently they went around in a circle. 2.19. Filler words and phrases (you know, like, well, I mean, okay, oh well, anyway, yeah), lnterjections when they do not convey lnfonnation about the content of the picture(s) or topic (oh, oh boy, wow, gosh, gee, golly, aha, hmm), and tag questions (it is really smashed up, isn’t it). 2.20. The conjunction “and” “And” is never counted as a correct information unit because it is often used as a filler and we have found that its use as filler cannot be discriminated reliably from its use as a conjunction. 2.21. Commentary on the task and lead-in phrases that do not give information about the picture(s) or topic and are not necessary for the grammatical completeness of the statement. 0 These pictures are poorly drawn. a This is kind of hard. 0 In the first picture... 0 As I said the last time, she was upset. 2.22. Commentary on the subject’s performance or personal experiences. a I can’t think of the name of that. - I can’t say it. No, that’s not right. My kids were always getting into trouble too. My wife and I used to fight like that. They are fighting but I don’t know why. Some statements that contain personal information may be appropriate in procedural and personal information descriptions and, in such cases, they would be counted as correct lnfonnation units (if they meet the other criteria). See 3.16 for embellishments that are counted as correct information units. See previous page for statements that are deleted before beginning the word and correct information unit counts. 3.1. @UNT THE FOLLOWING (if they meet all other criteria) (In this section, words in bold print would be counted as correct information units.) 3.11. All words (nouns, adjectives, pronouns, verbs, adverbs, articles, prepositions, and conjunctions) that are intelligible in context, accurate in relation to the picture(s) or topic, and relevant to and informative about the content of the picture(s) or topic. 3.12. Words do not have to be used in a grammatically correct manner to be counted. Words that violate standard English grammar rules concerning appropriate verb tense and form, agreement in number between subject and 92 predicate, agreement between articles and nouns, incorrect use of articles, and appropriate singular and plural forms are counted as correct lnfonnation units unless these violations would lead to misunderstanding or uncertainty about the meaning of the words. See 2.11 for examples of words that would not be counted as correct information units. The firemans are comin. The firemen ain’t rescued them yet. Put some stamp on it. The friends is here. He don’t look very happy. 3.13. Production of a word that results in another English word, if the production would be intelligible as the target word in context. a He is standing on a school and it is tipping over. 3.14. The final attempt in a series of attempts to correct sound errors. 0 He went to the musket...minuet...market. 3.15. Informal terms (nope, yep, uh-huh) when they convey information about the content of the picture(s) or topic. 0 She said “Uh-huh, I’ll do it.” 3.16. Words in embellishments that add to the events portrayed in the picture(s) or express a moral, if they are consistent with the situation or events portrayed. Words that express some legitimate uncertainty about characters, settings, or events in the pictures. 0 He’s going to get hurt and his mom is going to be angry. Some days everything seems to go wrong. That looks like a nice way to spend a summer day. Sooner or later cats usually get stuck up a tree. Mothers sometimes get distracted and don’t notice things. 0 This is the one about the accident-prone family. However, see 2.22 for examples of extraneous commentary that may resemble embellishments, but are not counted. 3.17. Verbs and auxiliary verbs (is, are, was, were, to, has, have, will, would, has been, etc) as two separate correct information units - one for the auxiliary verb and one for the main verb. 0 His mom is going to be angry. (Each word in bold print is a correct information unit.) 3.18. Contractions [both standard (won’t) and colloquial (gonna)] as two correct information units. 3.19. Each word in hyphenated words (father-in-law, good-bye). BIBLIOGRAPHY 92 BIBLIOGRAPHY Affolter, F., Bischofberger, W., 8 Calabretti-Emi, V. (1996). Nonverbal intergction in bppies and bflin-damaged patients. Poster session presented at the symposium: The growing mind — La pensee an evolution, Geneva, Switzerland. Affolter, F., & Bischofberger, W. (2000). Nonverbal perceptgal and cognitive proce§ses in children with language disorders. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Anonymous Author. (1999). Rehabilitation of persons with traumatic brain injury. The JouLmal of the A_merican Medical Association, 282, 974-983. Basso, A., Spinnler, H., Valler, G., & Zanobio, M. (1982). Left hemisphere damage and selective impairment of auditory verbal short-term memory: A case study. Neuropsychologia, 20 (3), 263-274. Ben-Yishay, Y., Diller, L. (1983). Cognitive deficits. In M. Rosenthal, E.R. Griffith, M.R. Bond, & J.D. Miller (Eds.), Rehabilitation of the head-injured adult (pp. 167-183). Philadelphia, PA: PA Davis Company. Bloom. L. (1996). The integration of expression into the stream of everyday activity. Lecture at Michigan State University’s Symposium: Movement and Action: Links to Intelligent Behavior, East Lansing, Michigan. March, 1996. Brooks, UN (1972). Memory and head injury. The Jogmal of Nervofius and Mental Disease, 155(5), 350-355. Brooks, UN. (1975). Long and short term memory in head injured patients. Cortex 11 329-340. Brooks, N. (1984). Cognitive deficits after head injury. In N. Brooks (Ed.), Closed head injury: Psychological. social, and family conseguences (pp. 44-71). Oxford, Great Britain: Oxford University Press. Brookshire, RH. (1997). Traumatic brain injury. In R. Brookshire (ed), Introduction to neurogenic commgnication disord_eg (5th ed .), (pp. 326-387). St. Louis, MS: Mosby-Year Book, Inc. O< Cicerone, K., & Wood, J. (1987). Planning disorder after closed head injury: A case study. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation 68,111- 1 15. Chapey, R. (1994). Assessment of language disorders in adults. In R. Chapey (Ed.), Language intervention strategies in adult aphasia (3rd ed.), (pp. 80-120). Baltimore, MD: Williams & Wilkins. Ewert, J., Levin, H., Watson,M., 8r Kalisky,Z. (1989). Procedural memory during posttraumatic amnesia in survivors of severe head injury. Aghives of Negrolgqv. 46, 911-916. Freund, J., Hayter, C., MacDonald, S., Neary, M., & Wiseman-Hakes, C. (1994). Cognitive communication disorders following tragmatic brain inilfl A practical guide. Tucson, AZ: Communication Skillbuilders. Gazella, J. (1997). Children’s stonuetelling in different modality conditions. Unpublished masters thesis, Michigan State University, East Lansing. Gazella, J. 8 Stockman, l. (2003). Children’s story retelling under different modality and task conditions: Implications for standardizing language sampling procedures. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 12, 1-12. Gillam, R.B., Cowan, N., & Marler, J.A. (1998). Information processing by school-age children with specific language impairment: Evidence from a modality effect paradigm. Journal offipeech. Language and Hearing Research 41, 913-926. Gillis, R.J. (1996). Traumatic prain injuiy rehabilitation for speech- Ianguage pathologists. Boston, MA: Butterworth-Heinemann. Glenburg, A. (2002). Reading and action: Manipulation enhances reading comprehension and memory. lntemational Symposipm for ChM Language & Symmsium fpr Research on Child Language Disorders. Glenburg, AM. (1997). What memory is for. Behavioral and Brain Sciences (1997), 20, 1-55. Godfrey, H., Knight, R., Marsh, N., Moroney, B., 8 Bishara, S. (1989). Social interaction and speed of information processing following very severe head-injury. Psychological Medicine, 19, 175-182. 06 Green, 3.8., Stevens, K.M., 8r Wolfe, TD. (1997). Mild traumatic brain iniugy: A therapy and resource manual. San Diego, CA: Singular Publishing Group Inc. Groher, M. (1977). Language and memory disorders following closed head trauma. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 20, 212-223. Gronwell, D. & Wrightson, P. (1981 ). Memory and lnfonnation processing capacity after closed head injury. Journal of Neurolpgy, Neurosurgegy, and Psvcllairv. 44, 889-895. Gross, G. M. (2001). Origami. New York, NY: Michael Friedman Publishing Group, Inc. Gmen, AK, 8 Gruen, LS. (1994). Daily problem solving activities: flapsjtjoningto independence. Eau Claire, WI: Thinking Publications. Heller, M.A., & Schiff, W. (1991 ). Psychology of touch. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Hinckley, J.J. (1999). Differential outcomes of functional and _ngpropsvchological approaches to naming treatment among adplts with chronic nonflgent aphapig, Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Michigan State University, East Lansing. Huitt, W. (2003). The information processing approach. Educational Psycholpgy Interactive [On-line]. Kreutzer, J.S., & Wehman, RH. (1991). Cognitive rehabilitation for persons with traumatic brain iniI_Irv: A functional approach. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brooks Publishing Co. Latham, S. 8 Stockman, l. (2001, November). Learning as a result of event participation: A comparative study: Learning as a function of event participation. Poster session presented at the annual American Speech- Language Hearing Association Convention, New Orleans, LA. Levin, HS. (1992). Neuropsychological rehabilitation of head injured patients: An appraisal of recent progress. Scandinavian Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine (Suppl. 26), 14-23. Lezak, MD. (1979). Recovery of memory and Ieaming functions following traumatic brain injury. Cortex 15 63-72. Marler, J.A., Champlin, C.A., & Gilliam, RB. (2001). Backward and simultaneous masking measured in children with language-Ieaming impairments who received intervention with Fast ForWord or Laureate Ieaming 97 systems software. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 10, 258-268. Moore Sohlber, M., & Mateer, CA (2001). Cpgnitive rehabilitation: An intpgrative neuropsycholpgical approach. New York, NY: The Guilford Press. Moore Sohlberg, M., & Mateer, C. (1989). Introduction to anitive rehabilitation: Theom and practice. New York, NY: The Guilford Press. , Murray, L.L, Ramage, A.E., & Hopper, T. (2001). Memory impairments in adults with neurogenic communication disorders. Seminars in Speech and Language,22(2), 127-136. Neistadt, ME. (1994). The neurobiology of Ieaming: Implications for treatment of adults with brain injury. The American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 489(5), 421-430. Neistadt, ME. (1994). Perceptual retraining for adults with diffuse brain injury. The American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 48(3), 225-233. Nelson, K. (1986). Event knowledge: Structure and function in development. Hillsdale, New Jersey: Erlbaum. Nicholas, LE, 8 Brookshire, RH. (1993). A system for quantifying the inforrnativeness and efficiency of the connected speech of adults with aphasia. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 36, 338-350. Rosenthal, M., Griffith, E.R., Bond, R.M., & Miller, JD. (1990). Rehabilitation of the adult and child with traumatic brain injug (2“d ed.). Philadelphia, PA: F.A. Davis Company. Stemberg, R.J. (1996). Memory Processes. In R. Stemberg (Ed.), ngnitive Psychology (pp. 252-278). Forth Worth, TX: Harcourt Brace College Publishers. Stemberg, R.J. (1996). Memory: Models and structures. In R. Stemberg (Ed.), Cognitive Psychology (pp. 222-250). Fort Worth, TX: Harcourt Brace College Publishers. Stockman, l. & Latham, S. (2001, November). The sensory input variable in Ieaming novel verbal/nonverbal tasks. Poster session presented at the annual American Speech-Language Hearing Association Convention, New Orleans, LA. Stockman, I. (2000). Epilogue: Language Ieaming and nonverbal interaction in daily events. In F. Affolter & W. Bischofberger (Eds), Nonverbal perceptual and cognitive processes in children with language disorders (pp.197-246). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. 98 perceptual and cognitive processes in children with language disorders (pp.197-246). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Streri, A., & Pecheux, MG. (1986). Tactual habituation and discrimination from infancy: A comparison with vision. Child Development 57, 100-104. Timmennan, M.E.,& Brouwer, W.H. (1999). Slow information processing after very severe closed head injury: Impaired access to declarative knowledge and intact application and acquisition of procedural knowledge. Neuropsychologia, 37, 467-478. Tomasello, M. (1995). Pragmatic context for early verb Ieaming. In M. Tomasello& W. Mern'man (Eds), Beyond Names for Things: Young Children’s Acguisition of Vera Hillesdale: Erlbaum. Ylvisaker, M., & Szekeres, S. (1998). A framework for cognitive rehabilitation. In M. Ylvisaker (Ed.),Trapmatic brain injury rehabilitation: Children and adolescents (pp. 125-136). Boston, MA: Buttenworth-Heinemann. Ylvisaker, M., Szekeres, S.F., & Haarbauer-Krupa, J. (1998). Cognitive rehabilitation: Organization, memory, and language. In M. Ylvisaker (Ed.), Traumatic Brain lnjgrv Rehabilitation: Children and Adolescents (pp. 181-220). Butterworth-Heinemann: Boston, MA. 00