.; .3.“qu .a . w , ~ .fl. . . . . , .. ht. aim? . . . . .. . , 3.2. .5 « a ._ a w. m. .meb... 5.3.». , . .m. 1:.) f n .3". . . £0: IA... r] 1.! ‘ J Law THE?“ 200 3 617*}; o'g’t‘l 3 This is to certify that the thesis entitled FACTORS INFLUENCING THE ADJUSTHENT 0F KOREAN CHILDREN WHO LIVE IN THE 0.8. presented by Sun-J in 011 has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for Mien—degree in Whip-ant Major professor Date {”5”} /1 320033 \] / 0-7639 MSU is an Affirmative Action/Equal Opportunity Institution LIBRARY Michigan State University PLACE IN RETURN BOX to remove this checkout from your record. To AVOID FINES return on or before date due. MAY BE RECALLED with earlier due date if requested. DATE DUE DATE DUE DATE DUE 6/01 c-JCIRCIDatoDuo.p65-p.15 FACTORS INFLUENCING THE ADJUSTNIENT OF KOREAN CHILDREN WHO LIVE IN THE US. By Sun-Jill Oh A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University In partial fulfillment of the requirements For the degree of MASTER OF ARTS Department of Family and Child Ecology 2003 ABSTRACT FACTORS INFLUENCING THE ADJUSTMENT OF KOREAN CHILDREN WHO LIVE IN THE US. By Sun-J in Oh The purpose of this study was to identify factors influencing the adjustment of Korean children who live in the US. Specifically, this study examined the following predictor variables: English proficiency, peer relationships, family relationships, and school experiences. Forty seven Korean children who were attending the Korean Language School and their parents participated in this study. Pearson Product Moment Correlations indicated that there was a statistically significant relationship between the adjustment of Korean children who live in the US. and their English proficiency and school experiences. There was no statistically significant relationship between the adjustment of Korean children who live in the US. and their family relationships and their peer relationships. However, the relation between adjustment and peer relationships approached significance with a two-tailed test and was significant if a one-tailed test was used. Additionally, according to the results of a regression analysis, English proficiency did not significamly predict the adjustment of Korean children when school experiences were controlled. However, the effects of English proficiency on adjustment were mediated through the school experiences. School experiences had a direct effect on the adjustment of Korean children who live in the US ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS I would like to express my appreciation to Dr. Thomas Luster, my advisor and chairperson of my committee, for his tireless effort, boundless patience, and ceaseless encouragement. He has taught me to understand the most valuable lesson in the true meaning of hard-working. His positive influence will have a profound impact on both my personal life and my future career. Thank you for being there, Dr. Luster. I would also like to thank the members of my committee, Dr. Harriette MaAdoo, for her valuable comments and assistance to carry this project though to the end. I thank Dr. Darrell W. Meece, for his comments. I would like to thank my mother, Soonja, Hawg, my father, Jeyang Oh, and my brother, Dushik Oh, for their never-ending love, and support throughout my life. Without them, I would not be able to go through all these years here. I also thank for my sister, brother-in—law, grandmother, for their concern and prayers. I would like to express my gratefirlness to my senior, Sojung Seo, for her constant encouragement and academic support throughout my stay at Michigan State University. I am truly thankfiil to the Korean parents and their children who volunteered to participate in this project. Without them this project would not be possible. III I». TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES .......................................................................... vi LIST OF FIGURES ........................................................................ vii CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION ............................................................................ 1 Statement of the Problem ..................................................................... 2 Significance of the Study ..................................................................... 2 Conceptual Model ............................................................................. 3 Research Questions ............................................................................ 5 Conceptual and Operational Definitions .................................................... 6 Research Assumptions ........................................................................ 7 CHAPTER 11 REVIEW OF LITERATURE ................................................................ 8 Children’s perceptions of adjustment .............................................. 8 English proficiency ................................................................... 9 Peer relationships .................................................................... 12 Family relationships ................................................................. 13 School experiences .................................................................. 16 CHAPTER III RESARCH METHODOLOGY .............................................................. 21 Research Desrgn ................. 21 Research Sample ............................................................................... 21 Research Instruments .......................................................................... 22 The Adjustment Scale ................................................................ 22 An English Language Proficiency Index ........................................... 23 Peer Relationships Scale ............................................................. 24 Family Relationships Scale ........................................................... 24 School Experiences Scale ............................................................ 25 Data Collection Procedure .................................................................... 25 Data Analyses ................................................................................... 26 CHAPTER IV RESULTS ....................................................................................... 27 Demographic Characteristics of the Sample ....................................... 27 Relations among the total score of the Parents’ ratings about the children’s adjustment and Family Demographic Variables ............................ 30 Relations between the adjustment of Korean children and independent Variables ......................................................................... 32 The relation between the predictor variables and the parents’ ratings of their children adjustment ............................................................. 35 IV Multiple predictors of the adjustment of Korean children who live in the US ............................................................................... 35 Summary of Results ........................................................................... 39 CHAPTER V DISCUSSION AND CONCLUSIONS .................................................... .42 Limitations ...................................................................................... 47 Implication for the future research .......................................................... 49 APPENDICES..............., ................................................................. 51 Appendix A: Parental Consent Form ........................................................ 51 Appendix B: Children Survey Questionnaire .............................................. 53 Appendix C: Parent Survey Questionnaire ................................................. 59 Appendix D: UCRIHS Approval ............................................................ 63 BIBLIOGRAPHY .............................................................................. 65 Table 1: Table 2: Table 3: Table 4: Table 5: Table 6: LIST OF TABLES Family Demographic Characteristics ................................................. 28 T-Test for differences between two groups of birth place in the total adjustment score of the parents perceptions about their children’s adjustment ................................................................................ 30 One-way AN OVA results for parents’ perceptions about their children’s adjustment ................................................................................ 31 T-Test for differences between two groups of primary language with parents in the total adjustment score of the parents perceptions about their children’s adjustment ............................................................ 32 The relation between the predictor variables and the parents’ ratings of the adjustment ................................................................................ 35 Multiple Regression Analysis: Predictors of the adjustment of Korean children who live in the US ...................................................................... 36 LIST OF FIGURES Figure 1: Possible influence on the adjustment of Korean children in the US ......... 5 Figure 2: Path model with a mediating variable ........................................... 38 VII CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION Culture, a very powerful force in young children’s lives, shapes representations of childhood, values, customs, child-rearing attitudes and practices, family relationships and interactions. Rodd (1996) said that children’s development could be fully understood when it is viewed in the larger cultural context. When individuals move from their native culture to another, they invariably experience environmental and psychological stresses. Children living in the US. after living in another culture and society not only face the initial stress and expected adjustment problems of all children, but also face an entirely different cultural environment. The problems, including language barriers, difficulties in social connections and networks such as peer relationships and interactions, and cultural differences, can discourage their efforts to adapt to the new social environment and culture. According to the 1990 US. Census, children and adolescents under age 20 rmde up 34.9% of the total number of Koreans living in the US. Among them, 67% were born in the US. Because of their parents’ immigration to study or work, minority children often came into or were born in the US. They may suffer from adjustment stresses in school and inadequate communication between school and parents. In addition to other losses, they experienced leaving their friends, extended family, and all the familiar surroundings of their home land (Kim, Kim & Rue, 1997). Stresses faced by Korean children in an alien culture would be even greater because of their lack of English proficiency and the cultural differences between Korea and America (Koh & Koh, 1988; Ho, 1992a). However, many minority children are able to cope with these problems and achieve competence and find satisfactory ways to adjust to a new cultural environment. Therefore, the concern of this study is factors that are associated with the positive or negative adjustment of Korean children who live in the US. Because Chinese and Japanese have represented the largest Asian populations in the US, most studies of Asians in the US. are based on their samples. There is a paucity of Korean studies in general, but even more scarce are data on Korean children who live in the US. Thus, this study will explore the current status of Korean children in America and will contribute to the literature related to them. Statement of the Problem The purpose of this study is to identify factors influencing the adjustment of Korean children who live in the US. Adjustment is defined as “a function of the degree to which the environment fulfills a person’s needs and goals and it is reflected directly in feelings of satisfaction with various areas of life” (Taft, 1973, p. 20 reported in Taft and Steinkalk’s 1985 study). Specifically, this study will investigate different factors influencing the adjustment of Korean children who live in the US, by examining the following variables: English proficiency, peer relationships, family relationships, and school experiences. Significance of the Study First, this study offers practical guidelines to Korean parents who want to help their Korean children adjust successfully to life in the US. This study can encourage parents to seek more ways to help their children succeed in American society. In addition, this study is usefirl to teachers who have Asian students in their classes. To increase effective teaching practices, it is crucial for teachers to understand how different variables, such as language and choices of peers, influence student behavior and learning. Also, the results of this study could be used to help policy makers assist Asian students. Conceptual Model Ecological Perspective The theoretical fiamework is adopted from an ecological perspective. In the ecological perspective proposed by Bronfenbrenner (1979, 1986), “the developing person is viewed not merely as a human being on which the environment makes its impact, but as a growing, dynamic entity that progressively moves into and restructures the milieu in which it resides” (p 21). The ecology of human development involves the process of the interaction between a person and the environment. The ecological environment is conceived as an arrangement of structmes such as the microsystem, mesosystem, exosystem, and macrosystem. Ho (1992b) suggested the following four levels to assess minority children, based on the ecological perspective: (1) At the individual level, the assessment is focused on the bio-psychological endowment of each child, including physical appearance, personality strengths, cognition, problem-solving skills, level of acculturation and language skills. (2) The family-level focus is on family-life style, immigration history, culture, family organization, gender-role structure, affective styles, and traditions. (3) At the cultural level, the focus should be on understanding the value systems, belief systems, and societal norms of the main-stream culture. (4) At the environmental level, the assessment should be based on understanding the economic, social, and political structures of American society that discriminate and oppress individuals in minority groups. According to the ecological perspective, a child’s problems or difficulties are “understood as a lack or deficit in the environment, as adaptive strategies such as culture shock and conflict” (Ho, 1992b, p 17). If children adjust well in and interact effectively with their current environment, their problems may disappear. Therefore, the assessment of the interaction between the child and the environment, such as family, school, and peer group, is important in research on ethnic minority children. If there is a mutually beneficial interaction between minority children and their environment, this should improve their ability to adjust to a new culture and to adapt to stressful social conditions (Ho, 1992a). This study, shown as in Figure 1, will examine portions of the child’s microsystems. A microsystem is a pattern of activities, social roles, and interpersonal relations experienced by the developing person in a given face-to-face setting (Bronfenbrenner, 1994). That is, this study will investigate the interaction among these systems potentially affecting the adjustment of Korean children who live in the US. The independent variables such as English proficiency, peer relationships, family relationships, school experiences, and the length of stay in the US. my influence the adjustment of Korean children who live in the US. English proficiency Peer relationships The adjustment of Korean children who / live in the us. School Family relationships Figure 1. Possible influence on the adjustment of Korean children in the US. Research Questions 1. Is there a relationship between the adjustment and the English proficiency of Korean children who live in the US? 2. Is there a relationship between the adjustment and the family relationships of Korean children who live in the US? 3. Is there a relationship between the adjustment and the peer relationships of Korean children who live in the US? 4. Is there a relationship between the adjustment and the school experiences of Korean children who live in the US? 5. Do school experiences mediate the relation between English proficiency and the adjustment of Korean children? 6. Do peer relationships mediate the relation between English proficiency and the adjustment of Korean children? 7. Do school experiences mediate the relation between family relationships and the adjustment of Korean children? Conceptual and Operational Definitions Dependent Variable The following section provides the conceptual and operational definitions of the dependent variable. The dependent variable in this study is the adjustment of Korean children who live in the US. Conceptually, adjustment of Korean children refers to Korean children’s subjective feelings of being in harmony with their environment, while living in the US. Operationally, the adjustment of Korean children will be measured by asking Korean children to complete the Adjustment Scale that include the following subscales: Desire to Reside in the US, Self-esteem, Depression Scale, and Self-confidence (Hightower, 1987; Simpson, 1992; Rumbaut, 1994; Eshel & Rosenthal-Sokolov, 2000). Indemndent Variables The following section provides the conceptual and operational definitions of the independent variables which are English proficiency, peer relationships, family relationships, school experiences, and the length of stay in the US. Conceptually, English proficiency refers to Korean children’s self-reported ability to speak, understand, read, and write English. Operationally, English proficiency will be measured by asking Korean children to complete the English Proficiency Index (Rumbaut, 1994). Conceptually, peer relationships refer to Korean children’s self-reports about how they feel about their relationships with peers and acceptance by peers. Operationally, peer relationships will be measured by asking Korean children to complete a modified version of the Interpersonal Social Skills measure (Hightower et al., 1987). Conceptually, family relationships refer to Korean children’s perceptions about whether the relationships with their parents are characterized by warmth, control, or conflict. Operationally, family relationships will be measured by asking Korean children to complete a modified version of the Parent-Child Conflict Scale (Rumbaut, 1994) and the Family Warmth measure (V ergne, 1982). Conceptually, school experiences refer to Korean children’s subjective feelings of satisfaction with their relationship with teachers, the school system, and their academic success. Operationally, school experiences will be measured by asking Korean children to complete a modified version of the School Experiences measure (Simpson, 1992). Research Assumptions These are the following assumptions related to this research: 1. Children’s beliefs and behaviors are influenced by the current culture in which they live. 2. Korean children from first grade to 6th grade are generally able to understand the instructions and items given to them to measure their adjustment. CHAPTER H REVIEW OF LITERATURE Children’s perceptions of adjustment Taft (1973 as reported in Tafi and Steinkalk’s 1985 study) defined adjustment as a function of the degree to which the environment fulfills a person’s needs and goals and it is reflected directly in feelings of satisfaction with various areas of life. Where the person is generally satisfied with life one might expect a feeling of well-being which would be reflected in emotional stability, competence in dealing with the environment, and a positive self-concept (p. 20). Adolescents who successfully integrate and accept their past culture with their current culture, and successfully own their cultural roots, are considered adjusted in the host society (Eshel, 2000; James, 1997). According to James (1997), adjustment and adaptation occur through the process of acculturation, not assimilation. “Assimilation” and “acculturation” are different. Assimilation usually forces people to give up valuable beliefs and behaviors to become part of the mainstream culture. For example, children and adolescents face pressure to assimilate into the new culture, acting as if the past culture never existed (James, 1997). Conversely, in acculturation, children and adolescents change their behavior, social and work activities, thinking patterns, values, and self-identification as a result of contact with the host society culture, without discarding past meaningful traditions and values (Eshel, 2000; James, 1997). If children and adolescents can be helped to accept both cultures, they can develop an integrated sense of self (James, 1997). Vergne (1982) studied the positive adjustment of 45 foreign-born children ages 6 to 10 to a new school and culture. He pointed out that the child’s attitude towards school, friends and peers, and to the US, maintenance of ethnic/national origin identity, and length of stay in the US. were all related to cultural adaptation. Particularly, the child’s attitudes towards fiiend and peers were significantly correlated with a few variables such as self-esteem, child’s attitude towards the US, and the length of stay in the US. Moreover, the length of stay in the US. not only correlated with school adjustment but also with self-esteem, the child’s attitude toward school, and home adjustment (Vergne, 1982) Rutter (1987, cited in Canino & Spurlock, 2000) suggests four specific coping mechanisms in children that act to protect against stress among children exposed to a new culture, that is: (1) the ability to reduce the impact of stress, (2) avoid negative chain reactions, (3) establish self-efficacy and self-esteem, and (4) seek new opportunities. Epglish mgficiemy It is important to assess the degree of fluency in English because this factor may affect competence in schoolwork or relationships with family and peers. The lack of English proficiency exacerbates virtually every problem area of Asian Americans (Ho, 1992a). Asian Americans’ general lack of proficiency in the English language and problems of miscommunication still may occur because the Asian culture’s de-emphasis of verbal skills in meaningful interpersonal exchanges and because they do not have parallel vocabularies or may not know various meanings of words (Ho, 1992a; Ho, 1992b) Children who are not fluent in the language of the host country may experience degrees of culture shock in the school setting. They may become depressed, confused, and hopeless about fitting into the American peer groups, if they do not understand the language well. James (1997) mentioned that even when a child has learned conversational aspects of the second language, it might take more than five years, on average, to learn those aspects of language involving cognitive functioning and academic achievement. According to Canino and Spurlock (2000), in a school setting, lack of English proficiency is rarely considered as a possible cause for achievement difficulties, and frequently a child undergoing the acquisition of a second language is diagnosed incorrectly as a learning-disabled child. They mentioned that children become alienated from school when the classroom teacher points out that minority children’s linguistic style is inferior to Standard English. Korean American children reared by primarily Korean—speaking parents may have difficulties with socialization in the new culture and early learning experiences (Ho, 1992a; Ho, 1992b; Kim, Kim & Rue, 1997). Parents tend to depend on their children to speak for them in situations requiring fluent English because children and adolescents are often more fluent in English than their parents. James (1997) said tlmt this practice results in role reversal, placing both the parent and child in difficult positions. The child, as the interpreter, is exposed to some information reserved for parents. Parents may become angry because of both their dependence upon and the involvement of the child in adult business. Consequently, these children are confused since they must constantly switch 10 fi-om an adult’s role to a child’s role and back and feel obligated to learn English quickly to help their parents (James, 1997; Chiang, 2000; Bhattacharya, 2000). Ethnicity is experienced and persists through language. Even though a native language provides a psychic bond or uniqueness that influences membership in an ethnic group, many minority children’s school problems and Emily conflicts are related to language problems (Ho, 1992b). If the language is different between children and their parents, the generational and cultural rift may widen within Asian American families (Ho, 1992a). Cheng et a1. (2000) said that “families which cherish cultural heritage and ethnic cultural values seem more likely to require that their children preserve ethnicity by learning the Emily language and thus put greater pressure on minority children” (p. 465). They noted, citing Portes’s 1994b study, that first generation immigrants are usually quite loyal to their ancestral languages. However, second generation immigrant children are often asked to speak their mother tongue at home and communicate with others in English outside of the Emily. They seem to experience more problems and conflicts in family relationships, as well as social connections and networks. Yu and Kim (1983) suggested that social reinforcement and parental encouragement are necessary for bilingual and bicultural children to develop both languages in verbal and written form without apparent loss of proficiency in either language. Bilingual children have the advantage of being able to establish a more positive ethnic self-identity and self-acceptance as well as being able to appreciate bicultural enrichment in later years. 11 Peer relationships Some Korean children may perceive their physical appearance as short and small, with black hair and black eyes and may recognize their skin color as ugly, inferior or shameful. This negative perception may be reinforced by peers who tease them. Also, this perception may have an effect on their personality or adjustment in an unfamiliar environment (Yu & Kim, 1983). Peer association is a common experience of children and adolescents across cultures. Social networks that children establish and maintain with peers may play a significant role in social support for children to cope with emotional stress and adjustment difficulties. According to the study of Chen et a1. (2001), peer sociability makes positive contributions to children’s social and emotional development and school adjustment beyond the impact of socioeconomic status. In contrast, peer aggression contributes to children’s learning problems and is negatively related to children’s school competence. They mentioned that the contributions of peer social functioning to children’s adjustment might be due to the direct contact and mutual influence among group members. Constant peer evaluations and reactions, based on culturally prescribed group norms and values, may serve to reguEte and direct children’s behaviors and, thus, affect normal developmental processes (Chen et al., 2001). In studies of children’s self-perceptions of abilities, researchers have found that elementary school-aged children were able to accurately assess their competencies in various domains, as well as to assess their peer acceptance (Cillessen & Bellmore, 1999). Children who lack meaningful interactions with peers will not have the information that is necessary to make accurate judgments about themselves. Therefore, to acquire the 12 formation and refinement of adaptive behavior, especially in the school setting, children’s interactions with others are necessary (Cillessen & Bellmore, 1999). They found that children with poor peer relations (rejected and controversial children with peers) had low self-other agreement scores. Namely, self-understanding may depend on the quality of interactions and relationships with significant others. Children who were more accurate at perceiving which peers liked them most were also more accurate at perceiving who liked them least. However, children correctly identified twice as many peers who liked them as who disliked them (64% vs. 35%) (Cillessen & Bellmore, 1999). Aboud (1988) mentioned, based on studies of White rmjority children’s attitudes toward other groups, that White children as young as 3 years of age expressed negative attitudes toward Blacks. Prejudice among 4 year olds becomes considerably more prevalent, and is well documented. Three to five year-old White children often choose a Black person as looking ‘bad,’ as having negative qualities or as being least preferred as a playmate. Particularly, over 2/3 of children from 5 to 7 years claimed that an Asian, Black, or Native Indian person is bad or disliked (Aboud, 1988). Famin relationships Korean parents tend to get involved positively in many kinds of activities with their children, especially academic performance. They don’t hesitate to sacrifice everything to provide a good educational environment for their children. Many Korean children in the US. live with both their biological parents. Such stable Emily life positively influences the children in the training of growth-promoting values and emotional stability (Yu & Kim, 1983). 13 According to Fagen et a1. (1996), parental warmth is positively related to children’s self-image, social competence, and self-regulatory abilities. They indicated that children who perceived their parents as warm have better social skills and meaningful interpersonal relationships. Because children have internalized social roles and regularities, they can better ask for support and help from relevant others in stressful situations (Fagen et al., 1996). Sun (1992a) measured perceived parental control and conflict with their parents, the level of adolescent’s acculturation, and many other variables with 83 Korean American adolescents. She found that the Korean adolescent’s acculturational variables had a significant relationship with maternal control and conflict. The author assumed that the Korean-American mothers who were less acculturated than their children could pressure their children to maintain aspects of the Korean culture, even though Korean American Ethers may emphasize bicultural ability because most fathers were employed in American society (Sun, 1992a). The parents’ attitudes toward their heritage in a dominant society are important in the child’s cognitive functioning and mental health by emphasizing their culture of origin in children’s attitudes and behavior, especially in competition with non-supportive socializing agents such as the school (Ou & McAdoo, 1999). They found that parents’ attitudes toward Chinese culture, language usage with the child, and the length of stay in the US. were correlated with the child measures, such as self-concept, racial/ethnic attitudes, and Chinese language ability. Parents whose young children use both Chinese and English showed more positive attitudes toward Chinese culture than parents whose children mainly use English. Parents may assist children to balance their abilities in 14 English and Chinese because young children may feel the dual pressure of doing well in school and of keeping their native language. Gonzales et a1. (1995) studied the mother-daughter relationship for each of 60 Afiican American and Asian American families. Three aspects of the relationship were examined: maternal warmth/support, maternal restrictive control, and parent-adolescent conflict. Their research indicated that warmth and control in Asian American families correlated with the family’s level of acculturation. The more acculturated Emilies scored higher in warmth and lower in control (Gonzales et al., 1995). Okagaki and Frensch (1995) examined the beliefs of 82 parents OH“ and 5th grade Mexican American children, focusing on parents’ educational values and expectations, parents’ own ability to help their children with schoolwork, parents’ perceptions of racial barriers that their children might encounter in life, and many other measures. They found that the parents of high achievers in school achievement tests were more confident about their ability to help their children in school. Moreover, parents of high achievers disagreed with the racial barrier items more than did the parents of low achievers. They more strongly affirm that their children will not encounter barriers to their success in life because of their ethnicity. Consequently, Okagaki and Frensch (1995) suggest that parental help in understanding school tasks and in adapting to the social context of the classroom plays a significant role for minority elementary children in the United States. Immigrant parent-child conflicts are significantly increased in cases where the children prefer English and also have a poor command of their parents’ native language (Ho, 1992a; Sun, 1992a; Rumbaut, 1994). Asian children who live in the US. are 15 expected to obey parents and maintain family honor. On the contrary, dramatic and painful value conflicts with their parents emerge when American-born children defy and reject their ancestors’ way of living. Their significant trouble is often caused by balancing the demands of becoming Americanized according to society and the desires of the parents to adhere to the values of their ancestors (Ho, 1992a; Sun, 1992a; Sandhu, 1999; Kibria, 2002). Some Asian American children might think their parents’ expectations are unreasonable and at odds with the American way of living. In particular, they might speak disapprovingly of the psychological pressures and tactics exerted by their parents. This widened gap and intergenerational culture clash in the values of parents and children are a psychologically painful wound among all the members in immigrant Emilies (Sandhu et aL, 1999; Kibria, 2002). Children’s psychosocial adaptation is mostly shaped by the Emily context. The assimilation into the current culture is moderated by parental ethnic socialization, social status, and parent-child relationships. Parent—child conflict may emerge as the strongest determinant of poor self-esteem, depression, and isolation from their parents (Rumbaut, 1994; Sandhu et al., 1999). School emriences Children’s beliefs and behaviors are influenced by school experiences. The importance of school as an institution for the socialization of children is considered by some to be second after the Emily. School is “an arem where minority children first experience cultural conflict and behavioral adjustment problems” (Ho, 1992b, p. 20). The American school system can influence the educational achievement and behavioral 16 adjustment of an Asian child (Ho, 1992a; James, 1997). The impact of social change as part of the acculturation process is most likely to be experienced by foreign-born children in the school setting. A lack of acceptance by peers and teachers may foster a sense of being different (James, 1997). By group discussions with Puerto Rican adolescents and their mothers, Fennelly, Mulkeen and Giusti (1995) found that both mothers and daughters were concerned that their teachers’ lower expectations for Latino youth stemmed from a view that Puerto Rican students were incapable of doing well in school. They cited a comment of one girl who participated in the discussions. She said, “American teachers put us down; they talk to us like we’re not gonna make it. They do not believe that Puerto Ricans can succeed” (p, 374). Several adolescents added that teachers equate difficulty with English with a lack of intelligence. Several of the mothers perceived that White teachers punish students differently based on their ethnicity. Ogbu (1987, cited in Fennelly et aL’s 1995 study) mentioned that the relationship between minorities and White Americans who control the public schools is often characterized by conflict and distrust. Parents of Asian American children suggested that academic achievement was a way of overcoming the racial exclusions and barriers in the United States. They said that the message to do well at school was offered as a racial strategy, a way of overcoming the obstacles of race. Doing well at school was presented as an effective means to achieve socioeconomic status and rewards in the United States (Kibria, 2002). Bhattacharya (2000) examined the school adjustment process of 75 school children and their parents who had immigrated to the US. fi'om Bangladesh, India, and Pakistan. He identified some Ectors that protect children of all ethnic groups fiom l7 dropping out of the school. The first Ector is a strong link between children’s parents and teachers as two primary socialization agents. Their congruent message about the importance of education Ecilitates children’s school adjustment. The second Ector is parents’ positive expectations which encourage children to have goals, and to recognize that education is the mechanism for helping them acquire the right direction in life. Also, Bhattacharya (2000) revealed several barriers to the school adjustment of South Asian children living in the US. He insisted that pressure fi'om parents to uphold the Emily’s reputation and guilt over the Eilure to firlfill family expectations may cause low self- esteem or the desire to associate with deviant peers. The teaching format designed to provide a better education for White American children was based on the American mainstream curriculum with little awareness of cultural diversity (Chiang, 2000; Canino & Spurlock, 2000). They note that teachers and parents tend to differ on specific values, such as self-direction or conformity in children’s education and guidance. Such value disparities between parents and school staff may affect the quality of their relationship and may increase the likelihood of interpersonal conflicts between teachers and children who come from other culture. These conflicts, in turn, may result in lower school achievement, a lower self-concept, and higher school dropout rates among foreign-born children (Ho, 1992a; Kim, Kim & Rue, 1997; Canino & Spurlock, 2000). Canino and Spurlock (2000) suggest that the classroom teachers should acknowledge and appreciate children’s home cultures, offering opportunities for children to express individual and cultural differences. The curriculum should include the cultural and racial diversity of our society. The school should provide children an opportunity to 18 learn skills that are not parts of their native culture but that are necessary to achieve practical success in the US. Additionally, social class is another dimension that describes and defines the child’s world by ascribing a particular position and value to his Emily’s socioeconomic status. A child or adolescent from a low-income Ernily will experience a restricted range of opportunities, choices, and challenges in particular social contexts (Ho, 1992b). The length of residence in the US. and the developmental stage of children when the Emily immigrates will affect their adjustment and development in the new environment. Kim, Kim and Rue (1997) mentioned that the longer the child lives in the US, the more he or she is acculturated. That is, the younger the age at immigration, the higher the level of the imrnigrant’s assimilation into the English language and American culture. For example, a 9 year old is more easily acculturated than a 15 year old. However, the younger the age at immigration, the less likely the child is to develop true bilingualism and biculturalism. Rong and Preissle (1998, cited in Chiang’s 2000 study) studied Asian American children and found the following: (1) Increasing length of US. residence reduces dropout rates among youth who have developed oral English proficiency: (2) Among Southeast Asian children from low-income Emilies, the bilingual speaking children have significamly lower dropout rates than monolingual English speaking children fi'om the same region. To summarize, prior research provides support for the view that the adjustment of Korean children living in the US. is likely to be influenced by several factors. The 19 factors include: English language proficiency, family relationships, peer relationships, and school experiences. CHAPTER HI RESARCH METHODOLOGY Research Design This study was designed to investigate Ectors influencing the adjustment of Korean children who live in the US. In order to carry out the objectives of this research most effectively, a non-experimental survey research design was used. It was a cross- sectional and local study in nature. The unit of analysis in this study was the Korean children from the first grade to the sixth grade in elementary school and their Korean parents. The age of the children ranged from 61 months to 156 months. Surveys were administered to Korean children who are attending the Hanmaeum Korean Language School and were sent to their parents in the greater Lansing, MI area. Research Sample The primary subjects in this study were Korean children living in the US, and their parents. All children of Korean parents were collectively referred to as “Korean children,” whether they are American-born or Korean-bom, as long as they have two Korean biological parents. Lists of all Korean children and their parents were provided by the Hanmaeum Korean Language School located in Lansing, Michigan. Korean children who are attending the Hanmaeum Korean Language School on Sunday are also attending American public schools during the week. Prior to contacting any subjects, the research proposal was reviewed by The University Committee on Research involving Human Subjects (U CRIHS). Upon approval of the protocol, the investigator contacted the principal and teachers of the Korean Language School and explained the purpose of the study in order to acquire lists of Korean parents and children who met the qualifications of this study. Then, the investigator directly contacted Korean parents and children who agreed to participate in this study and visited their home. Over 90% of the Emilies who were contacted participated in the study. The researcher was able to keep track of how many surveys were distributed and how many surveys were returned. Research Instruments The following research instruments or adapted versions of these instruments were used in this study: (1) Adjustment Scale, (2) English Proficiency Index (Rumbaut, 1994), (3) Interpersonal Social Skills (Hightower et al., 1987), (4) Parent-Child Conflict Scale (Rumbaut, 1994) and Family Warmth (V ergne, 1982), and (5) Shool experiences (Simpson, 1992). The Adjustment Scale (outcome variable) The perceived cross-cultural adjustment scale was compiled by the investigator for this study. This measures incorporates items that were selected from the following measures: 1) Desire to Reside in the host culture designed by Eshel (2000), 2) Child’s self-esteem developed by Simpson (1992), 3) Depression Scale developed by Rumbaut (1994), and 4) Self-confidence designed by Hightower et al. (1987). The Desire to Reside in the host culture scale used Likert-types scales (1 = do not agree at all, 5 = totally agree) to investigate Russians’ subjective perceptions of adjustment to Israel (e.g., “I feel at home in Israel”). For this study, children responded to five items (e.g., I wish to live in the US). Possible responses ranged from strongly disagree to strongly agree. High scores on this scale indicate that children want to reside in the US. Child’s self-esteem, developed by Simpson (1992), was derived fi‘om the Family, Friends, and Self (F FS) Assessment Scales for Mexican American Youth. Self- 22 Esteem, which was origninally designed to assess the psychological status of youth, contains five items (e.g., How often do you think of things that you like about yourself?) Response scales for these items have the following scores: (0) never, (I) rarely, (2) sometimes, (3) often, and (4) almost always. High scores indicate high self-esteem. Depression symptoms, developed by Rumbaut (1994), were measured with a four-item subscale fiom the Center for Epidemiological Studies-Depression (CES-D) scale. The scale assess how often the respondents exprerienced symptomes of depression (e. g., “I felt depressed”) during the past week. Possible responses ranged from 1 = rarely to 4 = most of the time. High scores indicate that the Korean children were experiencing many depression symptoms. Self-confidence, designed by Hightower et al. (1987), measures perceptions of sureness about one’s school abilities. The scale contains eight items (e.g., I like to do schoolwork) and each item in the scale is scored fi'om usually no to usually yes. High scores on this measure indicate high levels of self-confidence. For the purposes of the current study, the perceived cross-cultural adjustment scale was revised to be suitable for Korean children. Cronbach’s apha for this measure was .83. An Epglish Lapwge Proficiency Index (wider variables) An English language proficiency index was developed by Rumbaut (1994), using four items measuring the respondent’s self-reported ability to speak, understand, read, and write English. Each item in the index is scored fi'om 1 to 4 (1 = not at all; 2 = not well; 3 = well; and 4 = very well), with an overall index score calculated as the mean of the four items. This English proficiency index was strongly correlated (.42, p < .0001) with the objective Stanford reading achievement test score, providing evidence of its validity. High scores indicate good English proficiency. 23 Peer Relationships Scale (predictor miglflefl Interpersonal Social Skills is one Ector from the Child Rating Scale (CRS) designed by Hightower et al. (1987). The CRS is composed of four Ectors: Rule Compliance/Acting Out, Anxiety/Withdrawal, Interpersonal Social Skills, and Self-confidence. The Interpersonal Social Skills scale is utilized to assess perceptions of interpersonal functioning and relationships with peers. The sample item for this study is “I have a lot of Korean fiiends.” For this study, children responded to seven items. Possible responses range from strongly agree to strongly disagree. High scores indicate that children have good interpersonal social skills (apha = .52). Famin Relationships Scale (prediptor variables) The Parent-Child Conflict Scale was developed by Rumbaut (1994). The scale consists of 3 items (e.g., My parents do not like me very much), with responses ranging fi'om 1 to 4 (1 = Not true at all, 4 = Very true). High scores indicate that conflict is high between parent and children. Family Warmth is a modified version of the Child’s Attitude towards Family Scale designed by Vergne (1982). The Child’s Attitude towards Family Scale originally consisted of nine items with an estimated reliability of .45. For this study, children responded to four items (e. g., My parents love me a lot). Possible 1 responses ranged from strongly agree to strongly disagree. High scores indicate that children more fiequently feel Emily warmth. Family Warmth and Parent-child Conflict items were combined to create an overall indicator of the children’s perceptions of Emily relationships. The reliability coefficient for this measure was .75. 24 Scmool Experienpes Scale (predictor variables) The School Experiences Scale was adapted by the investigator from the Family, Friends, and Self (F FS) Assessment Scales for Mexican American Youth which was developed by Simpson (1992). The School Experiences Scale (alpha = .79), which was originally designed to assess opinions about quality of school life and relationships with teachers, contains four itmes. For this study, children responded to five items (e.g., I have positive relationships with teachers). Possible responses ranged from strongly agree to strongly disagree. High scores indicate that children have good school experiences. Additionally, a Family Demographic Data questionnaire designed by the investigator was used to measure the length of stay in the US, the primary language spoken at home, primarily speaking language with their parents at home, occupation, and education. This measure was completed by the parents. Parents also provided information on the children adjustment. They were asked to fill out a questionnaire regarding their perceptions of their child’s adjustment to life in the US. They responded to eight items about the child’s satisfaction with his/her life in the US, peer relationships, teacher relationships, school experiences, and language proficiency (e.g., “He / She is satisfied with his/her life in the US”). Data Collection Procedure Data collection began on February 9, 2003 and ended on February 23, 2003. The Adjustment scale was administered to Korean children who were attending the Hanmaeum Korean Language School on Sunday. The investigator presented versions in two different languages (Korean and English) to aid the understanding of the young children. The survey questions were administered individually to first and second grade 25 students by the primary investigator. To better explain to them what each question is asking and what each answer is describing, the investigator presented a colorful card that included symbols representing each possible response (big blue circle and small blue circle, big red circle and small red circle). Each circle has a corresponding caption indicating strongly disagree, disagree, agree, or strongly agree. The first and second grade students were able to respond to each item by pointing to the appropriate circle to indicate how they felt about each question. The Parent Survey Questionnaire and Family Demographic Survey were sent to parents and were directly returned to the investigator who is a teacher at the Korean Language School. To encourage participation, the investigator directly contacted Korean children and their parents who met the qualifications of this study and visited them in their home. Each survey was identified by a code number for the purpose of the data analysis. All data were confidential. The final sample size was 47 Korean children and their parents. Data Analyses Descriptive statistics were used to analyze the Emily demographic data, such as the length of stay in US, parents’ educational level and occupational status Pearson Product Moment Correlation Coefficients was computed to analyze the relationship between adjustment as reported by Korean children and English proficiency, peer relationships, Emily relationships, and school experiences. A t-test was used to analyze a difference in parents’ perceptions of their child’s adjustment to life in the US. between children who spoke English and children who spoke Korean with their parents at home (ADD). Multiple regression analysis was used to determine which predictor variables were related to the outcome variable when the other predictor variables were controlled. 26 CHAPTER IV RESULTS Demographic Characteristics of the Sample Demographic characteristics of the sample were obtained by the Parent Survey Questionnaire which was administered to parents who have Korean children living in the US. A summary of sample characteristics is presented in Table l. Frequencies were run for each demographic variable, which gave the mean, standard deviation, and range. All of the children who participated in the research have attended Hanmaeum Korean Language School on Sunday, and both mothers and Ethers also participated. The data collected through the Parent Survey provided information about the child’s age, gender, birth place, the length of stay in the US, primary language at home, and communication language with parents. Additionally, the survey showed the education level and occupational status of both parents. The total subjects of this study consisted of 47 children and their parents. Of the children, 43% were male and 57% were female. The age of the children ranged fiom 61 months to 156 months with a mean age of 96 months. Of the parents who responded to the Parents Survey, 66% (n = 31) were mothers and 34% (n = 16) were Ethers. Of the Korean children who were participated in this study, 72% were born in Korea and 28% of the children were born in the US. The mean length of stay for the children in the United States was 57 months, with a range from 1 to 156 months. 27 Table 1. Family Demographic Characteristics (N = 47) Percent Mean SD Range Child’s age Child’s length of stay in the US. Child’s gender Child’s birth place Child’s primary language at home Communication language (between parents and child at home) Mothers’ educaLtion Less than bachelor’s degree With bachelor’s degree More than bachelor’ degree Fathers’ education With bachelor More than bachelor Mothers’ emploment status Unemployed Employed Father’s occupational status Unemployed Skilled worker 96 25.1 61-156 57 37.5 l-156 male 43% female 57% Korea born = 72% US born = 28% English 38% Korean 47% Bilingual 15% English 1 1% Korean 89% 2% 66% 32% 9% 91% 77% 33% 23.4% 23.4% Administrative personnel, small independent business 2.1% Business managers, proprietors of medium sized business 36.2% Major professionals 14.9% 28 In this study, 47% of Korean children were speaking Korean as their primary language at home; 38% of them were mostly speaking English, and 15% were speaking both Korean and English equally at their home. Also, 89% of the Korean children were primarily speaking Korean with their parents at home and 11% were speaking English with their parents. In regards to the parents’ education level, both parents of Korean children were relatively well educated people. Over 90% of the Ethers had more than a bachelor’s degree, such as a master’s or doctoral degree. As far as the education of the mothers was concerned, 66% of the mothers had a bachelor’s degree, while 32% had a master’s or doctoral degree. The research surveyed Korean parents whose children were attending the Korean language school. The Korean language school is affiliated with the Korean Student Organization at Michigan State University. Thus, most parents of the Korean children who participated in this study were students at Michigan State University. Koreans have great respect for learning and consider education as the mechanism for achieving goals and for fulfilling their dreams. It’s one of the main reasons why parents of Korean children move to America. In regard to the parents’ occupational status, 77% of the mothers were unemployed while 23.4% of the Ethers were unemployed. Specifically, 23.4% of the fathers were skilled workers; 2.1% of the fathers were administrative personnel, or small independent business owners; 36.2% were business managers, or proprietors of a medium sized business; and 14.9% were major professionals. Relations among the total score of the Parents’ ratings about the children’s adjustment and Family Demographic Variables The respondent parents were asked to fill out a questionnaire regarding their perceptions of their child’s adjustment to life in the US. They responded to questions about the child’s satisEction with his/her life in the US, peer relationships, teacher relationships, school experiences, and language proficiency. Bivariate correlations identified that the total score of the parents’ perceptions about their child’s adjustment to the US. was positively and significantly related to child’s length of stay in the US. Parents responded that the longer children have lived in the US, the better they have adjusted and acculturated in the US. (1' = .52, p < .01) In this study, 72% of Korean children were born in Korea and 28% were born in the US. A t-test showed that there was a significant difference in parents’ perceptions about their children’s adjustment between children who were born in Korea and children who were born in the US. Parents reported that children who were born in the US. were better adjusted than Korea born children. This finding is presented in Table 2. Table 2. T-Test for Differences between Two Groups of birth place in the total adjustment score of the parents perceptions about their children’s adjustment Birth Place Korea (n=34) U.S (n=13) t-value df P value. The total adjustment score 31 34 4.1 45 .01 Of the parents perceptions (2.6) (3.2) 3O Additionally, a one-way AN OVA was used to test for differences in adjustment scores among children who spoke Korean, English, or both languages at home. Of the total sample, 47% (n = 22) of Korean children were speaking Korean as their primary language at home; 38% (n = 18) were speaking English, and 15% (n = 7) were speaking both Korean and English equally. There was a significant difference among the three language groups in parents’ perceptions of their children’s adjustment. Parents reported that children who were primarily speaking English tended to be better adjusted in the US. This finding is presented in Table 3. Table 3. One-way AN OVA results for parents’ perceptions about their children’s adjustment Language Mean adjustments scores on the parents survey SD English (n=18) 33.22 “3" 3.1 Korean (n=22) 30.59 3.2 Both language (n=7) 30.43 1.5 Groups Mean Squares df f-value Prob. Between 40 2 4.5 .02“ Within 8.9 44 Note. Results of post-hoc test 3. English language group had higher adjustments scores than Korean language group b. English language group had higher adjustments scores than bilingual group 31 Also, a t-test identified that there was a significant difference in parents’ perceptions of their child’s adjustment to life in the US. between children who spoke English and children who spoke Korean with their parents at home. Of the total sample, 89% (n = 42) of the Korean children used Korean as their primary language with their parents at home, and 11% (n = 5) used English as their primary language with their parents (1 = 3.1, p < .01). Parents reported that children who used English as their primary language with their parents at home tended to be better adjusted in the US. This finding is presented in Table 4. Table 4. T-Test for Differences between Two Groups of primary language with parents in the total adjustment score of the parents perceptions about their children’s adjustment Children’s primary language with parents English (n=5) Korean (n=42) t-value df P value. The total adjustment score 35 31 3.1 45 .01 Of the parents perceptions (4.2) (2.8) Relations between the Adjustment of Korean children and independent variables Reseerch Question 1: Is there a relationship between the adjuit' ment and the English proficiency of Korean children who live in the US? For the outcome variable, children were asked to respond to questions about their adjustment, such as desire to reside in the US, self-esteem, depression, and self- confidence. Also, the Koren children filled out the English Proficiency Index (e. g., “How well do you speak English?”). Over 90% (n = 44) of Korean children responded “well or very well” on the question of “How well do you speak English?” A Pearson Product Moment Correlation indicated a statistically significant correlation between the 32 adjustment and the English proficiency of Korean children who live in the US. (I = .38, p -- .008). Research Question 2: Is there eieiationghip between the_ adjustment and the Emily giationships of Korean children who live in the US? Korean children were asked to respond to questions about their Emily relationships. The scale about Emily relationships was divided into two sub-scales: Family Warmth (e. g., “My parents love me a lot”) and Parent-Child Conflict (e.g., “I’m often in trouble with my parents because of our different ways of doing things”). A Pearson Product Moment Correlation was computed to see if there was a relationship between the two variables. The test showed no significant relation between the adjustment and the total Emily relationship scores of Korean children who live in the US. (r = .13, p = .386). Also, Pearson Product Moment Correlations were computed to see the correlations, based on sub-scale scores. The results showed no significant relations between the children’s adjustment and Family Warmth (I = .10, p = .51) and Parent-Child Conflict (I = .13, p = .40). Research Question 3: Is there a relationship between the adjustment and the at Whips of Koreen children wlp live in the US? Korean children were asked to respond to questions about their peer relationships (e. g., “I make fiiends easily”). A Pearson Product Moment Correlation showed no significant relation between the adjustment and the peer relationships of Korean children 33 who live in the US. (I = .26, p = .083). However the correlation approached significance; children who made friends easily tended to be better adjusted. Of the total sample, 36% (n = 17) of the Korean children scored higher than the mean score on the Interpersonal Social Skills scale. Notably, 80% of Korean children who had lived less than one year in the US. had a score that was lower than the mean score for the sample. They expressed some problems and difl’iculties making American friends. However, all the parents (100%) who participated in the Parent Survey responded “agree” (62%) or “strongly agree” (3 8%) to the item, “Your child gets along well with American friends.” Research Question 4: Is there a relatiopship between the adjustment and the school e5pe_riences of Koreep children who live iii—the U.S.? Korean children were asked to respond to questions about their school experiences (e. g., “I have positive relationships with teachers”). Of the total sample, 96% (n = 45) of Korean children in this study responded “strongly agree” or “agree” to the item, “I have positive relationships with teachers.” Also, 87% (n = 41) were satisfied with their school achievement. A Pearson Product Moment Correlation was computed to see if there was a relationship between these two variables. There was a significant relation between adjustment and the school experiences of Korean children who live in the US. (i = .59, p < .01). Children who reported positive school experiences tended to be well-adjusted. 34 The relation between the predictor variables and the parents’ ratings of their children adjustment The respondent parents were asked to fill out a questionnaire regarding their perceptions of their child’s adjustment to life in the US. They responded to questions about the child’s satisEction with his/her life in the US, peer relationships, teacher relationships, school experiences, and language proficiency. Pearson Product Moment Correlations showed no significant relations between the parents’ ratings of the children’s adjustment and the predictor variables (English proficiency, peer relations, and school experiences) completed by the children. However, school experiences were positively related to English proficiency and peer relationships. Refer to Table 5 for the results. Table 5. The relation between the predictor variables and the parents’ ratings of the adjustment The parents’ ratings English Peer School of adjustment Proficiency relations Experiences The parents’ ratings of 1.000 adjustment English Proficiency .27 1.000 Peer relations .03 .22 1.000 School Experience .13 .44“ .42" 1.000 Note: **p<.01 Multiple predictors of the adjustment of Korean children who live in the US. This section presents the findings of a regression armlysis that was done to determine which of the predictor variables (English proficiency, family relationships, peer relationships, and school experiences) are related to the adjustment outcome 35 variable, when the other predictor variables were controlled. The analysis demonstrated that school experiences were significantly related to children’s adjustment, while English proficiency, and Emily relationships were not related. Refer to Table 6 for the results. Table 6. Multiple Regression Analysis: Predictors of the adjustment of Korean children who live in the US. Outcome: The total adjustment scale score of the children Predictor variables Betas t-Statistic Prob. English proficiency .18 1.4 .18 Family relationships -.27 -l .9 .06 School experiences .66 4.4 .01 R sq = .42 F-Ratio 10.2 .01 Path analysis, which relies on multiple linear regression, attempts to isolate the separate contributions to an outcome variable made by a set of interrelated predictor variables. According to the results of the path analysis, English proficiency and Emily relationships did not significantly predict the adjustment of Korean children when school experiences were controlled. School experiences had a direct effect on the adjustment of Korean children who live in the US. However, the effects of English proficiency and family relationships on adjustment were mediated through school experiences. English proficiency and Emily relationships had an effect on school experiences, which in turn, predicted children’s adjustment. According to Baron and Kenny (1986), a given variable 36 may be said to function as a mediator to the extent that it accounts for the relation between the predictor and the criterion. Table 6 and Figure 2 present the results of multiple regression analysis and path analysis. The path model is consistent with the view that proficiency in English and geod relationships with Emily are important for positive experiences in school, and positive experiences in school contribute to good adjustments among Korean children living in the US However, a regression analysis indicated that peer relationships did not mediate the relation between English proficiency and the adjustment of Korean children. The analysis showed that peer relationships were not significantly related to Korean children’s adjustment. 37 033.8.» message m 53> ESE 53 .N 8sz S. V ms... .933 cosmeaeqfim 2m manumommooo fiem em: mmfimaowfiom ram. 323m 2 .\ :82an SEEM A moocotomxm mo Eofiamieae the. Begum a? wocomomoi eéwem wfi. Su__m_r_n_ary of Results This study was designed to examine Ectors influencing the adjustment of Korean children who live in the US. Specifically, this study examined the following variables: English proficiency, family relationships, peer relationships, and school experiences. Seven research questions related to the adjustment of Korean children were addressed. Pearson Product Moment Correlations were computed to see if there were relationships between the outcome variable and four predictor variables.Multiple regression analysis and path analysis were computed to determine which of the predictor variables are related to the adjustment outcome variable, when the other predictor variable was controlled. The level of significance chosen was .05. The results can be summarized in the following manner: 1. There was a statistically significant relationship between the adjustment of Korean children who live in the US. and their English proficiency. Children who were more proficient in English tended to be better adjusted. 2. There was no statistically significant relationship between the adjustment of Korean children who live in the US. and their Emily relationships. 3. There was no statistically significant relationship between the adjustment of Korean children who live in the US. and their peer relationships. However, this relation approached significance with a two-tailed test and was significant if a one-tailed test was used. Positive peer relations were associated with better adjustment. 4. There was a statistically significant relationship between the adjustment of Korean children who live in the US and their school experiences. Positive experiences in school were associated with better adjustment. 39 5. School experiences mediated the relation between English proficiency and the adjustment of Korean children. 6. Although children who were proficient users of English tended to have more American friends, peer relations did not mediate the relation between English proficiency and the adjustment of Korean children. 7. School experiences mediated the relation between family relationships and the adjustment of Korean children. Additionally, according to the results of regression analysis, English proficiency and family relationships did not significamly predict the adjustment of Korean children when school experiences were controlled. However, the effects of English proficiency and family relationships on adjustment were mediated through the school experiences. School experiences had a direct effect on the adjustment of Korean children who live in the US. Positive school experiences are associated with better adjustment among Korean children. Also, the parents’ perceptions of their child’s adjustment to the US. was positively and significantly related to the child’s length of stay in the US. Parents responded that the longer children have lived in the US, the better they have adjusted and acculturated in the US. A t-test showed there was a significant difference in parents’ perceptions about their children’s adjustment between children who were born in Korea and children who were born in the US. Parents reported that children who were born in the US. were better adjusted than Korea-born children. Also, a t-test identified that there was a significant difference in parents’ perceptions of their child’s adjustment to life in the US. between children who spoke English and children who spoke Korean with their parents at home. Parents reported that children who used English as their primary language with their parents at home tended to be better adjusted in the US. Additionally, a one-way AN OVA was used to test for differences in adjustment scores among children who spoke Korean, English, or both languages at home. There was a significant difference among the three language groups in parents’ perceptions of their children’s adjustment. Parents reported that children who were primarily speaking English tended to be better adjusted in the US. Pearson Product Moment Correlations showed no significant relation between the parents’ perceptions of their children’s adjustment and the other predictor variables (English proficiency, peer relations, and school experiences) completed by the children. 41 CHAPTER V DISCUSSION AND CONCLUSIONS This study’s purpose was to examine Ectors influencing the adjustment of Korean children who live in the US. Specifically, this study examined several of the child’s microsystems (the Emily, school, and peer groups). The independent variables, such as English proficiency, family relationships, peer relationships, and school experiences, might influence the adjustment of Korean children who live in the US. Four research questions regarding the adjustment of Korean children were addressed, and the results are summarized at the end of the previous chapter. The results from this study supported previous research in that two variables, English proficiency and school experiences, were significamly related to the adjustment of Korean children who live in the US. Prior studies investigated how the English proficiency of foreign-born children influences their acculturation and adjustment process in the host society (Ho, 1992a; Ho, 1992b; James, 1997; Bhattacharya, 2000). These studies concluded that limited English skills frequently prevented them fi'om interacting with other fiiends and from participating in various school activities. Over 90% (n = 44) of Korean children responded “well” or “very well” to the question of, “How well do you speak English?” Most of the Korean children who participated in this study have stayed longer than one year in the US, and 11% (N= 5) of the Korean children have lived in the US. for less than one year. The children who have stayed less than one year do not speak English fluently right now. However, they might master English in the near future and quickly adjust to the new culture because young children tend to learn foreign languages faster than adults. Currently, all children in this 42 study are attending a Korean language school in order to acquire and to maintain their original language. This finding is consistent with those of other studies (Ho, 1992a; James, 1997) which reported it is important to assess the degree of fluency in English and in the native Asian language because communication skills may be affecting competence in schoolwork or relationships with Emily and peers. Asian children may become depressed, confused, and hopeless about fitting into the American peer groups, if they do not understand the language well. Also, the parents who participated in this study expressed that the English skills of their children played an important role in the children’s acculturation into the mainstream culture. A one-way ANOVA was used to test the differences in adjustment among three groups of children: those speaking primarily Korean, English, or both languages at home. There was a difference among three language groups in the parents’ perceptions of adjustment. Parents reported that children who were primarily speaking English tended to be better adjusted in the US. Therefore, this finding is consistent with previous studies (Ho, 1992a; Ho, 1992b; Kim, Kim & Rue, 1997). These studies noted that Korean American children reared by primarily Korean-speaking parents may have difficulties with socialization in the new culture and early learning experiences. Many Asian children’s school problems and Emily conflicts are related to language problems. In this study, school experiences, such as teacher relationships, school achievement and grades, were significantly related to Korean children’s adjustment in the US. Korean children who had good school experiences have adjusted well to their new 43 environments. School-aged children are spending much time bonding with their peers and teachers at school. Thus, school adjustment is a very important Ector because it may enhance their life skills and make the acculturation process easier and Ester in the new country. Asian children tend to respect teachers as highly as their parents, viewing teachers as role models. Specifically, 96% (n = 45) of Korean children in this study responded “strongly agree” or “agree” with the item, “I have positive relationships with teachers”, and only 17% (n = 8) agreed with the item, “Some of my teachers are not very nice to me.” Therefore, this finding is consistent with previous studies (Ho, 1992a; James, 1997; Bhattacharya, 2000). They suggested that the impact of social change as part of the acculturation process is most likely to be experienced by foreign-born children in the school setting and a lack of acceptance by peers and teachers may foster a sense of being different. Additionally, the investigator ran a regression analysis to determine which of the predictor variables (English proficiency and school experiences) was related to children’s adjustment when the other variable was controlled. School experiences predicted adjustment in this analysis, but English proficiency was not related to adjustment when school experiences were controlled. The results of a path analysis showed that school experiences had a direct effect on the adjustment of Korean children who live in the US, and that the effects of English proficiency on adjustment were mediated through the school experiences. Children who had better English skills tended to have better school experiences. Moreover, children who had better school experiences tended to be better adjusted in the US. That is viewed as a key finding from this study. These results were supported by the research of Ho (1992b), James (1997) and Canino and Spurlock (2000). They stated that children who are not fluent in English may experience culture shock in the school setting, and they may be diagnosed incorrectly as learning-disabled children. Ho (1992b) noted that many Asian children’s school problems were related to language problems and emphasized that school was an arena where Asian children first experience cultural conflict and behavioral adjustment problems. Parents were asked to respond to questions about their children’s school adjustment through the Parent Survey. Surprisingly, 100% (n = 47) of parents agreed that their children had positive relationships with teachers, and they were satisfied with their children’s school achievement and grades. Also, only 15% (n = 7) of parents reported that their children had a difficult time adjusting to the US. schools. Even though this study did not have data on this topic, according to Bhattacharya (2000)’s study, Asian parents may recognize that the school is an important socialization agent in American culture. Thus, they are willing to participate in school meetings (parent-teacher conferences) and activities in order to improve their children’s academic performance. Probably, Asian parents tend to believe that the American educational system ensures all children an equal opportunity to benefit from public school instruction. Bhattacharya (2000) revealed several barriers to the school adjustment of South Asian children living in the US. He insisted that pressure fiom parents to uphold the Emily’s reputation and guilt over the Eilure to fulfill Emily expectations may cause low self-esteem or association with deviant peers. 45 Korean children were asked to respond to questions on the Family Relationships Scale. The scale was divided into two sub-scales: Family Warmth (e. g., My parents love me a lot) and Parent-Child Conflict (e. g., I’m often in trouble with my parents because of our different ways of doing things). A Pearson Product Moment Correlation showed no significant relation between adjustment and the Emily relationships of Korean children in this study (I = .13, p = .386). Korean children who were in the upper grades (fifth and sixth grade) tended to acknowledge conflict and dissatisfaction about their Emily relationships, while most Korean children who were in the lower grades were satisfied with their relationships with their parents. Specifically, 47% of Korean children in the upper grades had a score about the mean on the Parent-child Conflict scale, while 25% of Korean children in the first grade had scores above the mean on the Parent-Child Conflict Scale. Pressure from parents to achieve school success and to learn English or Korean quickly may cause alienation and loneliness. In regards to peer relationships, Korean children in this study were asked to respond to items such as “I make friends easily”. A Pearson product moment correlation showed no significant relation between adjustment and the peer relationships of Korean children who live in the US. However, 80% of Korean children who had lived less than one year in the US. had scores below the mean on the Interpersonal Social Skills scale. They expressed some problems and difficulties making American friends. However, all of the parents (100%) who participated in the Parent Survey responded “agree (62%)” or “strongly agree (38%)” with the item, “Your child gets along well with American fi'iends.” Surprisingly, most children in this study responded “strongly agree (49%) or “agree (40%)” to the item, “I have a lot of American fiiends.” A Pearson product moment correlations showed a significant relation between adjustment and having many American friends (1:: .43, p < .01). Having American fiiends also was positively related to English proficiency (I = .55, p < .01) and school experiences (I = .48, p < .01). Therefore, the result revealed that making American fiiends was an important Ector in the adjustment of Korean children. Positive peer relationships with American fiiends may play a pivotal role in children’s social and emotional development and school adjustment. Additionally, the parents’ perceptions of their child’s adjustment to life in the US. was significantly related to their child’s birth place and their length of stay in the US. In the present study, t-tests showed that there was a significant difference in the parents’ perceptions about their children’s adjustment between children who were born in Korea and children who were born in the US. Parents reported that children who were born in the US. were better adjusted than Korea born children. Also, parents reported, as studied by Kim, Kim and Rue (1997), that the longer children have lived in the US, the better they have adjusted. Limitations Although this research seemed to produce significant results that could be useful when studying Korean children as well as Asian-born children who live in the US, this study has sorrre limitations which need to be taken into consideration when studying or applying the results. First, all of the Korean children who participated in this study are currently attending the Korean language school in East Lansing, Michigan. Most of the 47 children who are attending the Korean language school have stayed more than one year in the US; only 11% (N= 5) of the Korean children had lived in the US. for less than one year. Therefore, many of them were speaking better English than Korean. Because of the sampling procedure, the sample is not likely to be representative of the Korean children who live in the US. The study had concentrated on the adjustment of Korean children attending the Korean language school. Thus, generalizations to the population of Korean children outside this environment should be limited. To obtain the most accurate informtion about the adjustment of Korean children who live in the U.S., a sample that reflects the heterogeneity of the Korean children in the US. is required. As expressed earlier, the Korean language school is affiliated with the Korean Student Organization at Michigan State University. Therefore, most parents of Korean children, especially the Ethers, who participated in this study were master’s or doctoral degree students at Michigan State University. Thus, their standard of living tended to be at a high level. If Korean children were selected at random throughout the state of Michigan, the results would have given a more accurate depiction of the population of Korean children who live in the US. The sample size was 47 subjects in this research. This is a relatively small sample. If the sample size was larger, there would be more power available to detect relationships among the variables of interest. Also, the survey questions were conducted on a one-to- one basis for the first and second grade students and colorful cards (e.g., big blue circle for strongly agree, big red circle for strongly disagree) were used to obtain responses from the younger children. 48 Although, the investigator assessed each of the young children individually, Korean children in the lower grades (first, second, and third grades) did not always understand the meanings of some items, especially, those in the Self-esteem and Self- confidence Scale. For example, it was difficult for them to answer questions such as “How often do you think of things that you like about yourself?” Also, the questionnaire had too many questions for the youngest children in the sample; they had difficulty concentrating on the task until all questions were answered. The youngest children tended to perform better on shorter questions also. Additionally, the instruments used in this study were developed for other ethnic groups, not for Korean children. The Korean children were the primary source of information for both the predictor variables and the outcome measure. Therefore, shared method variance could contribute to significant associations between predictor variables and outcome variables. Because of the paucity of literature on Korean children in the U.S., there was limited research to draw upon when the model was developed for this study. The literature which was reviewed for this study was often based on studies of Asian children from other countries. Implication for the future research The purpose of this study was to identify Ectors influencing the adjustment of Korean children who live in the US. Thus, the subjects who participated in this study were Korean children. However, Asian children who live in the US. are a heterogeneous group of many different Asian cultures (e.g., Japan, China, and the Philippines). The 49 factors influencing the adjustment of children from other Asian groups may be different. So, it would have been a much more informative study if data had been collected fi'om different ethnic groups across the US. It would be useful to collect data fiom multiple informants about the adjustment of Korean children in the US. Therefore, data from teachers will be needed in addition to questionnaires for children and parents. A questionnaire for teachers could assess the teachers’ experiences, opinions, and subjective appraisals of Korean children in their classes. It could measure the children’s leadership, academic achievement, learning problems, and school-related competence fi'om a teacher’s point of view. 50 APPENDIX A Consent Form 51 Parental Consent Form My name is SunJ in Oh I am a second grade teacher at the Hanmaeum Korean Language School, and I am also a graduate student at Michigan State University pursuing a Masters’ degree in Family and Child Ecology. I am curremly investigating Ectors influencing the adjustment of Korean children who live in the US. I am inviting you and your child to participate in the study and asking your permission to survey you and your child. Your child will be asked to respond to questions about their adjustment, English proficiency, Emily warmth, parent-child conflict, and school experiences. This survey will be conducted at the Hanmaeum Korean Language School that your child attends. Your child can decide to participate or not to participate in this study. The survey questions will take 10-15 minutes to complete. Parents will also fill a survey out. The survey for the parent assesses: parents’ perceptions of the child’s adjustment to life in the US. and background information about the Emily. You can fill out this survey and return it back to the investigator who is a second grade teacher at the Korean Language School. Your participation as well as your child is voluntary and you can discontinue your involvement or your child can discontinue his or her participation in this project at any time without penalty. All data will be kept confidential by the primary investigator. Your privacy will be protected to the maximum extent allowable by law. Your and your child’s responses will provide valuable information about Korean children living in the US. If you have any questions or concerns regarding the participation of yourself or your child feel flee to contact me, SunJin Oh, at (517) 355- 1241 or my advisor Professor Tom Luster, at (517) 432-3323. If you have questions or concerns regarding your rights as a study participant, or are dissatisfied at any time with any aspect of the study, you may contact-anonymously if you wish-Ashir Kumar M.D., Chair of the University Committee on Research Involving Human Subjects at MSU by phone (517) 355-2180, fax: (517) 432-4503, or regular mail: 202 Olds Hall, East Lansing, MI 48824. I/we voluntarily agree to participate in this study and give permission for my/our child/ren to also participate in this study. Child’s Name Parent’s Signature Date 52 APPENDIX B Children Survey Questionnaire 53 ID# Children Survey Questionnaire Hello, I am SunJ in Oh. I’m a second grade teacher in the Korean Language School. This survey is to identify factors influencing your adjustment while living in the US. The following questions have no right or wrong answers. Your responses will be confidential. Please, provide candid answers. For each question, check the one response. 1.Name 2. Grade in the Korean Language School Adjustment Scale 1) I wish to live in the US. l = Strongly disagree 2 = Disagree 3 = Agree 4 = Strongly agree 2) I wish to live in Korea soon. 4 = Strongly disagree 3 = Disagree 2 = Agree 1 = Strongly agree 3) I am satisfied with my life in the US. 1 = Strongly disagree 2 = Disagree 3 = Agree 4 = Strongly agree 4) I feel at home in the US. 1 = Strongly disagree 2 = Disagree 3 = Agree 4 = Strongly agree 5) I am unhappy about living in the US. 4 = Strongly disagree 3 = Disagree 2 = Agree 1 = Strongly agree 1) How often do you think of things that you like about yourself? 0 = Never 1 = Rarely 2 = Sometimes 3 = Often 4 = Almost always 2) Are you proud of how you act and the things you do? 0 = Never 1 = Rarely 2 = Sometimes 3 = Often 4 = Almost always 54 3) Do you think you have a lot to be proud of? 0 = Never 1 = Rarely 2 = Sometimes 3 = Often 4) Are you happy and satisfied with yourself? 0 = Never 1 = Rarely 2 = Sometimes 3 = Often 5) When you do something, do you think you do it well? 0 = Never 1 = Rarely 2 = Sometimes 3 = Often How often durmg' the m week: 1) I did not feel like eating; my appetite was poor. 4 = Rarely 3 = Some of the time 2 = Occasionally (1 or 2 days) (3 or 4 days) 2) I could not “get going.” 4 = Rarely 3 = Some of the time 2 = Occasionally (1 or 2 days) (3 or 4 days) 3) I felt depressed. 4 = Rarely 3 = Some of the time 2 = Occasionally (1 or 2 days) (3 or 4 days) 4) I felt sad. 4 = Rarely 3 = Some of the time 2 = Occasionally (1 or 2 days) (3 or 4 days) 1) I like to do schoolwork l = Usually No 2 = Sometimes 2) I’m relaxed when I am at school 1 = Usually No 2 = Sometimes 3) I feel calm when I am at school 55 4 = Almost always 4 = Almost always 4 = Almost always 1 = Most ofthe time (5 to 7 days) 1 = Most ofthe time (5 to 7 days) 1 = Most of the time (5 to 7 days) 1 = Most of the time (5 to 7 days) 3 = Usually Yes 3 = Usually Yes 1 = Usually No 2 = Sometimes 4) I like to answer questions in class 1 = Usually No 2 = Sometimes 5) I’m sure of myself at school 1 = Usually No 2 = Sometimes 6) I am unhappy at school 3 = Usually No 2 = Sometimes 7) I worry a lot when I am at school 3 = Usually No 2 = Sometimes 8) I don’t like being in school. 3 = Usually No 2 = Sometimes Independent Variables E_nglish Proficiency Index 1) How well do you speak English? 1 = Not at all; 2 = Not well; 3= Well; 4 = Very well 2) How well do you understand English? 1 = Not at all; 2 = Not well; 3= Well; 4 = Very well 3) How well do you read English? 1 = Not at all; 2 = Not well; 3= Well; 4 = Very well 4) How well do you write English? 1 = Not at all; 2 = Not well; 3= Well; 4 = Very well Peer Relationships Scale 56 3 = Usually Yes 3 = Usually Yes 3 = Usually Yes 1 = Usually Yes 1 = Usually Yes 1 = Usually Yes l) I have a lot of Korean friends 1 = Strongly disagree 2 = Disagree 3 = Agree 2) I have a lot of American friends 1 = Strongly disagree 2 = Disagree 3 = Agree 3) Classmates tease me 4 = Strongly disagree 3 = Disagree 2 = Agree 4) I wish I had more fi'iends at school 4=Stronglydisagree 3=Disagree 2=Agree 5) I wish I had more friends to play with in my neighborhood 4 = Strongly disagree 3 = Disagree 2 = Agree 6) I make fi'iends easily 1 = Strongly disagree 2 = Disagree 3 = Agree 7) I am often lonely. 4 = Strongly disagree 3 = Disagree 2 = Agree Family Warmth Scale 1) My parents and I do a lot of things together. 4 = Strongly agree 4 = Strongly agree 1 = Strongly agree 1 = Strongly agree 1 = Strongly agree 4 = Strongly agree 1 = Strongly agree 4 = Strongly agree 3 = Agree 2 = Disagree 1 = Strongly disagree 2) My parents love me a lot 4 = Strongly agree 3 = Agree 2 = Disagree 1 = Strongly disagree 3) My parents feel proud of me when I do something well 4 = Strongly agree 3 = Agree 2 = Disagree 1 = Strongly disagree 4) I feel happy at home 57 4 = Strongly agree 3 = Agree 2 = Disagree 1 = Strongly disagree Pmmt-CMLCoMiQSCfi: 1) I’m often in trouble with my parents because of our different ways of doing things. 4 = Not true at all; 3 = Not very true; 2 = Partly true; 1 = Very true 2) My parents are usually not very interested in what I have to say. 4 = Not true at all; 3 = Not very true; 2 = Partly true; 1 = Very true 3) My parents do not like me very much 4 = Not true at all; 3 = Not very true; 2 = Partly true; 1 = Very true _S__<_:_h_ool Experienees Scale 1) I have positive relationships with teachers 4 = Strongly agree 3 = Agree 2 = Disagree 1 = Strongly disagree 2) I am satisfied with my school achievement 4 = Strongly agree 3 = Agree 2 = Disagree 1 = Strongly disagree 3) I am getting good grades in school. 4 = Strongly agree 3 = Agree 2 = Disagree 1 = Strongly disagree 4) My teachers care about me. 4 = Strongly agree 3 = Agree 2 = Disagree 1 = Strongly disagree 5) Some of my teachers are not very nice to me. 1 = Strongly agree 2 = Agree 3 = Disagree 4 = Strongly disagree 58 APPENDIX C Parent Survey Questionnaire S9 Parent Survey Questionnaire Date: Thank you for taking time to complete this questionnaire. This is SunJin Oh. I’m a second grade teacher in the Korean Language School. This survey is to identify factors influencing your children’s adjustment while living in the US. The following questions have no right or wrong answers. Your responses will be confidential. Please provide candid answers. For each question, check the one response. Adjustment Scale of your child Directions: Read each statement and circle how you describe your child: 1) He / She is satisfied with his / her life in the US. Strongly agree Agree Unsure/don’t know Disagree 2) He / She gets along well with American children Strongly agree Agree Unsure/don’t know Disagree 3) He / She has positive relationships with teachers. Strongly agree Agree Unsure/don’t know Disagree 4) I am satisfied with his / her school achievement. Strongly agree Agree Unsure/don’t know Disagree 5) He / She has positive feelings about speaking two languages. Strongly agree Agree Unsure/don’t know Disagree 6) He / She would rather live in Korea Strongly agree Agree Unsure/don’t know Disagree 7) He / She has had a difficult time adjusting to the US. school. Strongly agree Agree Unsure/don’t know Disagree 8) He / She wishes he / she had more fiiends. Strongly agree Agree Unsure/don’t know Disagree Strongly disagree Strongly disagree Strongly disagree Strongly disagree Strongly disagree Strongly disagree Strongly disagree Strongly disagree Family Background Information I would like to close with some general important questions about your background. 1. Child’ name 2. Birth date b.) . Age of the child 4. Grade of the child 5. Sex of the child: Male Female 6. Your child was born in: Korea US 7. How many years have your child lived in the United States? years and months 8. I am the Mother Father 9. What is your child’s primary language at home? (1) At home he/she speaks only English (2) At home he/she speaks only Korean (3) At home he/she speaks mostly Korean, some English (4) At home he/she speaks mostly English, some Korean (5) At home he/she speaks both English and Korean, equally 10. Which language is usually spoken in your home with your children? (1) We speak only English at home (2) We speak only Korean at home (3) We speak mostly Korean, some English (4) We speak mostly English, some Korean (5) We speak both English and Korean at home, equally 11. Are you currently employed? (1) Yes (2) No 12. If yes, are you working full-time or part-time? (1) Full-time (2) Part-time 13. What is your job? (1) Unemployed (2) Unskilled worker (3) Skilled worker (4) Owner of small business, clerical, sales, technician 61 (5) Administrative personnel, small independent business, minor professionals (6) Business managers, proprietors of medium sized business (7) Executives and proprietors of large concern, major professionals 14. What is the highest level of forum] school that you have completed? (1) Less than High school (2) High school degree (3) Associate degree (4) Bachelor degree (5) Master degree (6) Doctorate / Professional (PhD, MD, JD, etc...) (7) Other (Please Specify) 15. Is your spouse/partner curremly employed? (1) Yes (2) No 16. If yes, is your spouse/partner working full-time or part-time? (1) Full-time (2) Part-time 17. What is your spouse/partner’s job? (1) Unemployed (2) Unskilled worker (3) Skilled worker (4) Owner of small business, clerical, sales, technician (5) Administrative personnel, small independent business, minor professionals (6) Business managers, proprietors of medium sized business (7) Executives and proprietors of large concern, major professionals 18. What is the highest level of formal school that your spouse/partner has completed? (1) Less than High school (2) High school degree (3) Associate degree (4) Bachelor degree (5) Master degree (6) Doctorate / Professional (PhD, MD, JD, etc...) (7) Other (Please Specify) 62 APPENDIX D UCRIHS Approval 63 OFFICE or RESEARCH ETHICS AND STANDARDS University Committee on Research lmrolvlng Human Subjects Michigan State University 202 Olds Hall East Lansing. MI 48824 517/355-2180 FAX: 517/4324503 Web: mmsuedu/user/ucrihs E-Mail: ucrihs@msu.edu The Michigan State University IDEA ls institutional Diversity. Excel/m in Action. MSU Is In aflimratiieaction. atrial-opportunity Instin/tion. MICHIGAN STATE UNIVERSITY February 6, 2003 TO: Tom LUSTER 13 G Human Ecology MSU RE: IRB# 03-040 CATEGORY: EXPEDITED 2-7 APPROVAL DATE: February 6, 2003 EXPIRATION DATEzJanuary 6, 2003 TITLE: FACTORS INFLUENCING THE ADJUSTMENT OF KOREAN CHILDREN WHO LIVE IN THE US. The University Committee on Research Involving Human Subjects' (UCRIHS) review of this project is complete and I am pleased to advise that the rights and welfare of the human subjects appear to be adequately protected and methods to obtain informed consent are appropriate. Therefore. the UCRIHS approved this project. RENEWALS: UCRIHS approval is valid until the expiration date listed above. Projects continuing beyond this date must be renewed with the renewal form. A maximum of four such expedited renewals are possible. Investigators wishing to continue a project beyond that time need to submit a 5—year application for a complete review. REVISIONS: UCRIHS must review any changes in procedures involving human subjects. prior to initiation of the change. If this is done at the time of renewal, please include a revision form with the renewal. To revise an approved protocol at any other time during the year, send your written request with an attached revision cover sheet to the UCRIHS Chair. requesting revised approval and referencing the project's IRB# and title. Include in your request a description of the change and any revised instruments, consent forms or advertisements that are applicable. PROBLEMSICHANGES: Should either of the following arise during the course of the work. notify UCRIHS promptly. 1) problems (unexpected side effects, complaints, etc.) involving human subjects or 2) changes in the research environment or new information indicating greater rlsk to the human subjects than existed when the protocol was previously reviewed and approved. If we can be of further assistance, please contact us at (517) 355-2180 or via email: UCRIHS@msu.edu. Please note that all UCRIHS forms are located on the web: http://www.msu.eduluser/ucrihs Ashir Kumar, MD. UCRIHS Chair AK: jm cc: sunjin ch 1449 Spartan Village APT H East Lansing, MI 48823 64 BIBLIOGRAPHY 65 BIBLIOGRAPHY Aboud, F. (1988). The Development of Ethnic Attitudes and Prejudice. In Frances Aboud, Children and prejudice, (chapters 3, 4). Oxford: Basil Blackwell. Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The ecology of human development: experiments by nature and design Cambridge, Harvard University Press. Bronfenbrenner, U. (1986). Ecology of the family as a context for human development: Research perspectives. Developmental psychology, 22, 723-742. Bhattacharya, G. (2000). The school adjustment of South Asian immigrant children in the United States. Adolescence, v. 35 no 137 (spring 2000), 77-85. Baron, R M. & Kenny, D A. (1986). The Moderator-Mediator Variable Distinction in Social Psychological Research: Conceptual, Strategic, and Statistical Considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51(6), 1173-1182 Canino, I. A. & Spurlock, J. (2000). The Influence of Culture and Multiple Social Stressors on the Culturally Diverse Child. In I. A. Canino & J. Spurlock, Culturally Diverse Children and Adolescents, (pp. 7-44). New York: Guilford Publications, Inc. Chen, X., Chen, H. K & Violet. (2001). Group social fimctioning and individual socioemotional and school adjustment in Chinese children. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 47(2), 264-299. Cheng, S. H & Kuo, W. H. (2000). Family socialization of ethnic identity among Chinese American pre-adolescents. Journal of Comparative Family Studies, 31(4). 463-484. Chiang, L. H. (2000). Teaching Asian American students. The Teacher Educator, 36(1), 58-69. Cillessen, A. H. & Bellmore, A. D. (1999). Accuracy of social self-perceptions and peer competence in middle childhood. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 45(4), 650-676. Eshel, Y. & Rosenthal-Sokolov, M. (2000). Acculturation attitudes and sociocultural adjustment of sojourner youth in Israel. The Journal of Social Psychology, 140(6), 677- 691. Fagen, D. B., Cowen, E. L. & Wyman, P. A. (1996). Relationships between parent-child relational variables and child test variables in highly stressed urban families. Child Study Journal, 26(2), 87-108. Fenelly, K, Mulkeen, P., & Giusti, C. (1995). Coping with racism and discrimination: The experience of young Latino adolescents. In H. I. McCubbin, E. Thompson, A. Thompson & J. Fromer (Eds), Resiliency in Ethnic Minority Families: Native and Immigrant American Failies (Vol. 1), Chap 18. Gonzales, N. A., Hivaga, Y. & Cauce, A. M. (1995). Observing Mother-Daughter Interaction in African-American and Asian-American Families. In H. I. McCubbin, E. Thompson, A. Thompson & J. Fromer (Eds), Resiliency in Ethnic Minority Families: Native and Immigrant American Families, (Vol. 2, p. 259 — p.285), Ho, M. K. (1992a). Asian American Children and Adolescent. In Man Keung. Ho (Ed.), Minority Children and Adolescents in Therapy, (pp. 33-54). Newbury Park, CA: SAGE Publications, Inc. Hightower, A. D., Cowen, E. L., Spinell, A. P., Lotyczewski, B. S., Guare, J. C. Rohrbeck, C. A. & Brown, L. P. (1987). The Child Rating Scale: The Development of a Socioemotional Self-Rating Scale for Elementary School Children. School Psychology Review, 16(2), 239-255. Ho, M. K. (1992b). A Transcultural Framework for Assessment and Therapy with Ethnic Minority Children and Youth. In Man Keung. Ho (Ed.), Minority Children and Adolescents in Therapy, (pp. 7-29). Newbury Park, CA: SAGE Publications, Inc. James, D C. S. (1997). Coping with a new society: the unique psychosocial problems of immigrant youth. The Journal of School Health, v. 67, 98-102. Kim, W. J., Kim, L. I. & Rue, D. S. (1997). Korean American Children. In Johnson- Powell, G. & Yamamoto, J (Ed.), T ranscultural Child Development: Psychological assessment and treatment, (pp. 183-207). New York: Wiley. Kibria, N. (2002). Becoming Asian American : second-generation Chinese and Korean American Identities. Baltimore, Maryland: The Johns Hopkins University Press. Okagaki, L. & Frensch, P. A. (1995). Parental Support for Mexican American Children’s School Achievement. In H. I. McCubbin, E. Thompson, A. Thompson & J. Fromer (Eds), Resiliency in Ethnic Minority Families: Native and Immigrant American Families, (Vol. 2), Chap 16. Cu, Y. S. & McAdoo, H. P. (1999). The Ethnic Socialization of Chinese American Children. In H. P. McAdoo (Ed.), Family Ethnicity: Strength in Diversity, (pp. 252-276). Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE Publications, Inc. Portes, P. R., Ziady, M. F. & Phalachandra, B. (1999). Cultural Differences in Self- Esteem: ethnic variations in the adaptation of recent immigrant Asian adolescents. In D. S. Sandhu (Ed.). Asian and Asian and Pacific Islander Americans: Issues and Concerns for Counseling and Psychotherapy. pp. 137-150. Commack, NY: Nova Science Publishers, Inc. 67 Rodd, J. (1996). Children, culture and education. Childhood Education, v. 72, 325-329. Rumbaut, R. G. (1994). The Crucible Within: Ethnic Identity, Self-Esteem, and Segmented Assimilation Among Children of Immigrants. International Migration Review, 28(4), 748-794. Sun, C. K. (1992a). Korean-American Adolescents: Parent-Child Relationship. Long Beach, CA: California State University. Simpson, D. D. & McBride, A. A. (1992). Family, Friends, and Self (FFS) Assessment Scales for Mexican American Youth. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 14(3), 327-340. Sandhu, D. S., Kaur, K. P. & Tewari, N. (1999). Acculturative Experiences of Asian and Asian and Pacific Islander Americans: Considerations for Counseling and Psychotherapy. In D. S. Sandhu (Ed.). Asian and Asian and Pacific Islander Americans: Issues and Concerns for Counseling and Psychotherapy. pp. 3-19. Taft, R. & Steinkalk, E. (1985). The Adaptation of Recent Soviet Immigrants In Australia. In I. R. Lagunes & Y. H. Pooninga (Eds), From a Different Perspective: Studies of Behavior Across Cultures, (pp. 19-28). Acapulco, Mexico: Swets & Zeitlinger B.V. Vergne, A R. (1982). A Study of the Positive Adjustment of Foreign Born Children to a New School and Culture. Michigan State University. Yu, K H. & Kim, L. I. C. (1983). The Growth and Development of Korean-American Children. In Johnson-Powell, G (Ed.), The Psychosocial Development of Minority Group Children, (pp. 147-158). New York: Brunner/Mazel, Inc. 68