I7} "L";$‘J‘:‘é 9(74 This is to certify that the thesis entitled THE EFFECTS OF ACCULTURATION ON ATTITUDES TOWARD CRIME AND JUSTICE AMONG INDIVIDUALS 0F MEXICAN DESCENT presented by Francisco Javier Barocio, Jr. has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for the MS. degree in Criminal Justice two ‘74; Major Professor’s Signature t-t-H- 0‘! Date MSU is an Affirmative Action/Equal Opportunity Institution IJBRARY Michigan State University PLACE IN RETURN BOX to remove this checkout from your record. TO AVOID FINES return on or before date due. MAY BE RECALLED with earlier due date if requested. DATE DUE DATE DUE DATE DUE imfihtgm: 6/01 c:IClRC/DateDue.p65-p.15 THE EFFECTS OF ACCULTURATION ON ATTITUDES TOWARD CRIME AND JUSTICE AMONG INDIVIDUALS OF MEXICAN DESCENT By Francisco Javier Barocio, Jr. A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF SCIENCE School of Criminal Justice 2004 ABSTRACT THE EFFECTS OF ACCULTURATION ON ATTITUDES TOWARD CRIME AND JUSTICE AMONG INDIVIDUALS 0F MEXICAN DESCENT By Francisco Javier Barocio, Jr. Acculturation theory suggests immigrants’ attitudes and cultural sensibilities change over time and slowly come to resemble those of the Anglo-American majority (Berry, 1997; Vigil, 1998). A similar process is hypothesized to take place among immigrant offspring. Through the acculturation process, immigrants and their children become more aware and accepting of American social control institutions and practices, and come to incorporate these mechanisms into their cultural and psychological repertoires. This research will examine the effects of acculturation on the attitudes toward family violence, youth deviance, and the police among individuals of Mexican- descent. Existing acculturation studies have relied on non-probabilistic and unrepresentative small samples, and have glossed over salient cultural differences. To address this shortcoming, the author uses community survey data collected in 1995 from the Project on Human Development in Chicago Neighborhoods in 1995. The data file includes 1,134 respondents of Mexican-descent. Bivariate and multivariate statistical techniques are used to assess directional hypotheses derived from acculturation theory. Dedico este tesis a mis padres, Francisco Barocio y Emma Uribe, que siempreformaran parte de mi vida iii ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS This thesis would not have been possible without the continued support, guidance and insights from Dr. Michael Reisi g. I thank him the utmost for his dedication, which makes him the best professor I have ever had in my academic career. I also would like to thank Dr. Perez McCluskey and Dr. Mahesh Nalla for their insightful comments and support. I am also grateful to the School of Criminal Justice at Michigan State University for introducing me to the academic field of criminology where I will continue to work to develop culturally informed theoretical frameworks. I would also like to thank Dr. James Diego Vigil, a professor in the School of Ecology at the University of California at Irvine, for his email correspondence regarding existing literature that I benefited from. I would also like to thank Dr. Israel Cuellar and Dr. Linda Hunt from the Julian Samora Research Institute at Michigan State University for providing me with information that was very helpful. On a more personal note, I would like to thank my five younger brothers and sisters - - Louis, Arturo, Roberto, Jacqueline, and Vanesa - - for thinking of them and encouraging me to my see that this project come to fi'uition and completion. Last but not least, I thank my cousin, Gerardo Magallanes, and my very close friend, Richard Lynn Mountz, for making me experience a wonderful life outside the academic sphere as well. Thanks for letting me know to keep both feet on the ground throughout this challenging enterprise, now finished. I expect more challenging ones in the future and the successes when they are overcome and complete. iv TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES .......................................................................... vi INTRODUCTION .......................................................................... 1 ACCULTURATION THEORY & RESEARCH ....................................... 2 Facets of Acculturation ............................................................ 7 Language ................................................................... 7 Ethnic Identity ............................................................. 8 ACCULTURATION THEORY & RESEARCH ........................................ 9 Attitudes Toward the Police ....................................................... 10 Attitudes ToWard Youth Deviance ............................................... 13 Attitudes Toward Family Violence ............................................... 14 RESEARCH OBJECTIVE .................................................................. 18 RESEARCH SETTING ..................................................................... 18 METHODS .................................................................................... 19 Data .................................................................................... 19 Dependent Variables ................................................................ 20 Independent Variables .............................................................. 22 RESULTS ..................................................................................... 24 Multivariate Analysis of Variance ................................................ 24 Ethnic Identity .............................................................. 26 Language .................................................................... 26 Acculturation ............................................................... 27 Ordinary Least-Squares Regression Analysis ................................... 28 Attitudes Toward the Police .............................................. 3O Attitudes Toward Youth Deviance ....................................... 31 Attitudes Toward Family Violence ...................................... 31 DISCUSSION ................................................................................ 32 APPENDIX ................................................................................... 38 REFERENCES ................... ' ............................................................ 4o LIST OF TABLES Table 1. Descriptive Statistics for the Study Variables (N = 1,134) ............... 21 Table 2. Principal Components Analysis ............................................... 23 Table 3. MANOVA Models .............................................................. 25 Table 4. OLS Regression Analysis ....................................................... 29 Appendix. Correlation Matrix for Independent Variables ............................. 39 vi INTRODUCTION Acculturation takes place in all societies, among all group members, regardless of cultural or social backgrounds. According to Redfield, Linton, and Herskovitz (1936, p. 149), acculturation concerns “those phenomena which result when groups of individuals having different cultures come into continuous first-hand contact, with subsequent changes in the original cultural patterns of either or both groups.” Immigrant and minority populations in the United States experience distinctive acculturation forms. Not only do such individuals encounter a majority Anglo-culture, they also assume different social positions resulting in an amalgam of cultural, social, and psychological adaptations (Berry & Annis, 1974; Berry, Kim, Minde, & Mok, 1987). The administration of justice, especially social control practices, can benefit from a better understanding of acculturation and its effects on attitudes toward criminality, deviance, and formal social control. Extant research has examined the effects of acculturation on delinquency, substance abuse, and intimate partner violence using self-report data (Firestone, Lambert, & Vega, 1999; Samaniego & Gonzales, 1999; Zayas, Rojas, & Malgady, 1998). Despite their contributions, these studies are not without limitations. For example, some studies have relied on non-probability and convenience sampling techniques (Vega, Khoury, Zimmerman, Gil, & Warheit, 1995; Wall, Power, & Arbona, 1993; Zule, Desmond, Medrano, & Hatch, 2001). This is a real concern because such procedures often yield results that cannot be generalized to larger populations. Another concern, especially in the area of intimate partner/ family violence, is the use of dichotomous acculturation measures, such as “language,” and crude ethnic markers (e. g., Latino or Hispanic) (Caetano, Schafer, Field & Nelson, 2002). These primitive ethnic categorizations disabuse the central tenets of acculturation theory (Ponce & Comer, 2003), and disservice the study of matters related to crime and criminal justice. | To fill the void in the literature regarding minority and disparate cultural populations, especially Mexicans, Mexican-Americans, and Chicanos, this project will investigate variation in attitudes toward the police, family violence, and youth deviance among individuals of Mexican-decent. Accordingly, this research will take into account intra-cultural differences, and also inform the debate regarding the need for culture- sensitive practices of formal social control. Finally, it is also h0ped that this project will augur well for the importance of cultural frameworks in criminology. To accomplish these objectives, the author will use community survey data collected in 1995 from “The Project on Human Development in Chicago Neighborhoods.” ACCULTURATION THEORY & RESEARCH Contemporary criminal justice systems in the United States exist within pluralist cultural settings. Traditionally, monolithic theories of crime and deviance have exerted too much influence in criminological research. This is unfortunate because such theories presume a unitary form of cultural acquisition across individuals (Myers, Cintron, & I Scarborough, 1994). Reliance on Eurocentric theoretical frameworks in criminological research has decreased (albeit slightly) over the past five decades (LaFree & Russell, 1993; Stanfield, 1993; Takagi, 1981). Despite this gradual change, our understanding of variations between Latino subgroups in criminology remains limited (Garcia, 1994). To contribute to the development of “cultural criminology,” this research seeks to move away from criminological theories that disregard differences within and between cultural groups. Cultural deviance theories (i.e., social disorganization, differential association, culture conflict, and subculture formation) promulgate cultural diversity wherein social classes exhibit different cultural mores and conflict with those of a unitary, middle-class, and predominately White culture (Adler, Mueller, & Laufer, 1991). While such frameworks take into account racial and ethnic diversity, they also suffer from the “fallacy of homogeneity” (Stanfield, 1993, p. 21). Put simply, these frameworks include overarching assumptions about uniform differences between cultural groups. Similar shortcomings can also be seen in official data sources compiled by the government. For example, the UCR and the NCVS recently began to include Latino ethnic identity classifications. Due to the prevalence of such ethnic identity markers as “Hispanic,” ’9 66 “Latino, other,” and “non-white,” these official statistics may perpetuate stereotypic notions of a unidirnensional culture (Myers et al., 1995). Padilla (1986) argues that this naming process of an all-encompassing Latino label, which includes Mexicans, Cubans, Puerto Ricans, as well as other Latino subgroups, lacks independent life and real existence. This research will inform criminal justice officials about variations that exist within Mexican culture specifically, and also reveal problems associated with using gross naming processes. To investigate the complexities of ethnic and cultural factors among individuals of Mexican-heritage, the author will examine the relationship between such factors and attitudes toward crime and justice. Specifically, previous concerns regarding the incompatibility of the “race” notion with Latino subpopulations - - an assumption that Black and White racial dichotomies have impinged upon Latino ethnic naming processes - - will be critically examined (see LaFree & Russell, 1993; Myers et al., 1994). The importance of cultural differences within Mexican-heritage groups can be studied using acculturation theory, which Olmeda (1979, p. 1061) deems “one of the more elusive, albeit ubiquitous constructs in the behavioral sciences.” Contact between Anglo culture and Mexican-oriented culture can occur firsthand or through the media (Berry, 1997; Redfield, Linton, & Herskovitz, 1936). In the context of the acculturation, Mexican-origin individuals’ minority status, when compared to the sociocultural and political majority status of Anglo-Americans, results in cultural and psychological changes (Berry, 1997). Mexican-descent individuals (unlike political refugees and indigenous people), according to Berry (1997), voluntarily immigrate into the United States and enter the acculturation process willingly. Individuals of Mexican-origin espouse three general degrees of acculturation that are saturated along the acculturation spectrum of Mexican, Mexican-American, and Chicano psycho-cultural identifications and orientations, all of which intimate the permanence or duration of their residence in the United States. Mexican-descent individuals choose from two general acculturating strategies during their everyday contact with Anglo-oriented culture: (1) cultural maintenance (e. g., maintaining Mexican identity and characteristics in daily interactions), or (2) contact and participation (e. g., involvement in non-Mexican cultural groups) (Berry, 1997). Further, Mexican-origin individuals in the United States select from four acculturation sub- strategies: Assimilation, integration, separation, or marginalization (Berry, 1997). One moderating factor prior to acculturation and eventual assimilation is “cultural distance,” which is defined as the extent of dissimilarity that exists between host cultures and the individuals in contact (Berry, 1997). Cultural distance helps explain why individuals of European-descent have historically acculturated more quickly than their Mexican-descent counterparts, and why the latter have been able to acculturate more rapidly than individuals of African-descent (Rosenfeld, 2001; Vigil, 1998). The experience of Mexican immigrants within the American assimilationist context (Berry & Sam, 1996; Berry, 1997; Vigil, 1997) - - also known as the host or “new” culture - - directly affects their adaptive and integrationist possibilities that could inhibit or facilitate acculturation and assimilation. Sociologists (e.g., Johnson, 2000; Rosenfeld, 2001) and some psychologists (e. g., Vasquez, 1984) have used the concept of acculturation synonymously with the concept of assimilation. Anthropologists (Kottack, 2002; Vigil, 1997, 1998) and intercultural psychologists (e. g., Berry, 1997; Fiske, 1996; Ward, 1996), on the other hand, differentiate between the two terms. Assimilation concerns the “process of change that a minority ethnic group may experience when it moves to a country where another culture dominates” (emphasis added) (Kottack, 2002, p. 298). In other words, assimilation is, by definition, a form of acculturation. Assimilation represents the last stage of acculturation, wherein the Chicano, in this case, would acquiesce completely to Anglo-culture, shedding completing any trace of Mexican culture whatsoever.l Because of the current l Acculturation must not be confused with enculturation. In the anthropological sense, enculturation is the process by which a child learns his or her culture (Kottack, 2002). For instance, a first-generation child living in a Mexican-oriented household will learn Mexican culture before being exposed to the majority and dominant Anglo culture. Enculturation is synonymous with the concept of socialization in cultural anthropology. Inter-cultural psychologists differentiate between socialization and enculturation (Berry, 1992). Socialization is defined as vertical social learning (culture learned from parents or guardians of study’s aim for cultural relativism and sensitivity, it sides with the anthropological interpretation of acculturation. Mexican-origin individuals settling in the United States experience deep cultural change, which cause subsequent changes in language use, religious beliefs and practices, ethnic identity, and alterations to preexisting value systems. These shifts are the product of three processes: culture shedding, culture learning, and culture conflict (Berry, 1992). The first two processes involve accidental or deliberate changes in behaviors, which allow the individual, to “fit” the Anglo culture. However, when individuals experience “fitting” conflict, acculturative stress ensues (Berry & Sam, 1996; Vigil, 1998), wherein they feel that intercultural contact cannot be dealt with by simply adjusting their beliefs and/or behaviors. In this research, acculturation is interpreted, not unlike Berry’s assessment (1997), as culturally and psychologically driven (Fiske, 1991; Ward, 1996). Several factors influence culture shedding in individuals who form part of a cultural minority group in the United States (i.e., Mexican, Cuban, Chinese, etc.). Females shed their original minority culture at slower rates than do males, thus decreasing the rate of biculturalism among the former and increasing the risk for them to conflict with their heritage culture once they further acculturate (Berry, 1997; Zentella, 1997). Schools or education represent important conduits and facilitators for more rapid acculturation (Vigil, 1997). Among immigrants, younger individuals (specifically 12 or younger) acculturate faster than do their older counterparts, as illustrated by the prevalent cultural gap between traditional Mexican parents and their Mexican-American children child), whereas enculturation represents horizontal learning (culture learned from extra-household agents, i.e., schools, media, peers, etc.). Nonetheless, socialization and enculturation has no direct relationship with acculturation (Vega et al., 1995). Class hastens the rate of culture shedding, namely higher class individuals are more acculturated than their lower class counterparts (Aycan & Berry, 1996). Locus of control, introversion/extraversion (Ward & Kennedy, 1992), and self- efficacy (Schwarzer, Hahn, & Schroder, 1994) are personality factors that also have been found to affect the course of acculturation. Migration motivation and expectations on the part of immigrants also directly influence acculturation outcomes (Richmond, 1993). Finally, the political context of the country may also be a salient factor. For instance, the United States and Canada are plural and multicultural societies wherein cultural groups are encouraged (or discouraged), and therefore retain more distinctiveness and self- identity (Berry, 1997; Kottack, 2002; Vigil, 1997). Facets of Acculturation Researchers have identified many facets of the acculturation process. Among these factors include language and etimic identity. It is important that research endeavors that attempt to investigate the effects of acculturation take these different facets into account. Language. Acculturation, by definition, represents forms of cultural processes and changes. Linguistic anthropologists argue that without culture, language represents an empty epiphenomenon (Duranti, 1994; Kroskrity, 1993; Zentella, 1997). In numerous studies language is specified as a major indicator of acculturation in that it accounts for most of the variance associated with acculturation (Cuellar, Arnold, & Maldonado, 1995; Marin, Sabogal, Marin, Ortero-Sabogal, & Perez-Stable, 1987). In short, both the theoretical and research literature supports the notion that language is a key component of acculturation. Many individuals of Mexican-origin, especially first and second generation, master both Spanish and English (Buriel, Perez, De Ment, Chavez, & Moran, 1998). Accordingly, language use fails to represent the all-encompassing indicator of acculturation, so another factor must also be identified and incorporated. Ethnic Identity. A second dimension of acculturation is ethnic identity. Because Mexican-descent individuals’ distinct physical appearance (Vigil, 1998), their perception of ethnic discrimination (Horowitz, 1983; Vigil, 1997, 1998), and their historical and continued presence in the United States (Chavez, 1998; Vigil, 1998), a strong social identity among individual of Mexican-origin has emerged and persisted. Nevertheless, heterogeneity exists among these social identities. Individuals of Mexican-origin trace their descent to Indian, African, and Spanish cultural groups. A mixture of all three groups, in turn, creates a unique cultural syncretism2 that manifests itself in individuals of Mexican-decent, what Vigil (1998, p. 141) calls “meztisaje.” Previous research supports the notion that a majority of first, second, third, and even fourth generation Mexican-Americans identify themselves as “Mexicans” and “Mexican-Americans” (Buriel & Cardoz, 1993; Phinney & Devich- Navarro, 1997). Acculturation levels can be gauged by different social identities. Research shows that those who identify themselves as “Mexican” represent those who have experienced the lowest level of acculturation (Cuellar, Amold, & Maldonado, 1995; Phinney, 1992). Those who adopt the “Mexican-American” ethnic label, in comparison, reflect medium levels of acculturation (sometimes referred to as “biculturalism”)(Buriel et al., 1998). 2 Syncretisrn, by definition, represents a more abstract phenomenon that anthropologists use to describe the intermingling and corrrmingling of cultures at the group level more so than the individual one. Cultures become mixed to form a hybrid “new” culture. This cultural process must not be confused with cultural diffusion wherein cultural attributes, are shared, and diffuse themselves vis-a-vis individuals to other geographic and disparate areas. The assumption is that culture is shared, learned, and transferred. Indicative of the highest level of acculturation is the “Chicano” ethnic label. Due to its difficulty to gauge, the latter ethnic descriptor has received the least amount of attention in the acculturation literature. Individuals who subscribe to a “Chicano” ethnic identity defend Mexican- American values, are politically active, and view their own culture as “better” than the majority Anglo-culture (Horowitz, 1983; Penalosa, 1973). Some criminologists use “Chicano” and “Mexican-American” interchangeably (Mirande, 1987; Perez-McCluskey, 2002; Trujillo, 1974). However, common to all definitions of the “Chicano” ethnic identity is its political charge (Meier & Ribera, 1993; Vigil, 1998). Arguably it exhibits pre-conditions attached to it (i.e., political involvement, advocacy, English proficiency, and US. citizenship), mitigating and lessening the cultural distance between Anglo- culture and Mexican-oriented culture. Nonetheless, Chicanos are ethnically different from white Americans. Taken together, language use and ethnic identity represent the most salient measures in the current study for distinguishing acculturation levels among Mexican- origin individuals. These items can be used to answer recent calls for the development of more sophisticated measures for acculturation, and assess the influence of acculturation on attitudes related to crime and justice. ACCULTURATION, CRIME, & JUSTICE “Studies on the impact of race or ethnicity in the criminal justice system have teIlded to focus on narrower issues such as bail determinations, jury selection, and sentencing decisions” (Garcia, 1994, p. 19). Criminological literature on gang activity, drug use, and violence among Latinos has been largely atheoretical (Rebach, 1992), acultural (Blea, 1988; Mirande, 1987), and empirically deficient (Garcia, 1994). Studies that do investigate how “Hispanics” view matters related to crime and justice are peripherally considered due to the same putative similar race-motivated disj uncture that Blacks (or all other “races”) experience with the criminal justice system (e. g., Sampson & J eglum-Bartusch, 1998). Some studies rely on problematic polling data (e.g., Di Rado, 1993). Other studies lack an adequate description of the methodological and the theoretical strategies implemented (Bedard, Escholtz, & Gertz, 1994; Palerrn, Vincent, & Vincent, 1999). In the present study, acculturation theory is used to explain attitudes toward crime and justice among Mexican-descent individuals. Attitudes Toward the Police Criminologists and police reformers argue that police agencies need to be more culturally relativistic and sensitive to the needs and values of the ethnic communities that they serve (Carter, 1988, 1985; Dunham & Alpert, 1988; Mirande, 1980; Safford, 1977; Tagaki, 1981). Tyler (1990) argues that police agencies that process and treat suspects fairly enjoy higher levels of police-civilian cooperation, as well as legitimacy in the eyes of local community members. Variation exists among individuals of Mexican-decent with regards to how they view the police. For example, newly immigrated Mexicans may be very suspicious of the police, whereas Chicanos may express more favorable views. Acculturation theory can be used to understand how individuals of Mexican-decent view agents of formal social control in the United States. Many observers have commented that corruption is widespread in Mexican political systems and among government officials (Buffington, 2000; Chevigny, 1995, 10 1999; Palerm et al., 1999). For example, Botello and Rivera (2000, p. 61) note that corruption in the Mexican government is “thoroughly institutionalized and operates at the local and state as well as federal levels.” Corruptive practices, such as accepting bribes, have also been observed among police officers (Chevigny, 1999). Focus groups composed of Mexican citizens conducted in one locale in Mexico revealed that citizens truly feared the police (Palerm et al., 1999, p. 80). The State Judicial Police is but one government agency that is frequently cited by international and domestic human rights organizations (Palerm etal., 1999). It is safe to conclude, then, that the Mexican police do not enjoy a high level of legitimacy in the eyes of Mexican citizens. Considerable cultural distance - - especially with regards to language and ethnic identity - - exists between the least acculturated Mexican-heritage individuals and Euro- Americans in the United States. Acculturation theory predicts that the least acculturated individuals, because of their distrust in police to begin with, will view police institutions in the United States less favorably when compared to their more acculturated counterparts. Several studies with representative samples in the US. reveal that Anglo- Americans, when compared to Blacks and Latinos, view police officials more positively, yielding higher levels of satisfaction (Reisig & Parks, 2000; Sampson & J eglum- Bartusch, 1999; Skogan, Steiner, DuBois, Gudell, & Pagan, 2002; Tyler, 1990; Weitzer & Tuch, 1999). Because of the cultural similarity between Chicanos and Euro-Americans in the United States, highly acculturated Mexican-origin individuals exhibit the most accepting orientation regarding the police. It should be noted that such an expectation is counter to Mirande’s (1987) contention that the sustained culture conflict inherent in the administration of j ustice in the United States among Mexican-descent individuals has a ll blanket effect, and will adversely influence attitudes among all individuals of Mexican- origin regardless of acculturation processes. For a variety of reasons, existing research has shed little light on whether acculturation theory or the culture conflict hypothesis provides a better understanding of Mexican-decent individuals’ attitudes toward the police. First, the sample sizes used in many studies are fairly small. For example, Torres and Vogel’s (2001) sample is limited to less than 63 “Latinos,” Mirande (1980, 1981) relies on 170 “Chicanos,” Garza’s (1974) sample includes only 68 “Chicano” participants, and Cheurprakobkit’s (2000) sample comprises 251 “Hispanics.” Second, the reliance on nonprobabilistic sampling techniques, by definition, produces samples that are not representative of larger populations. Even when probabilistic techniques have been used, other features of the sampling process call into question the generalizability of the researchers’ samples. For example, Carter (1983, 1985) drew a random probability sample of 500 potential respondents by selecting persons with Spanish surnames from a Department of Public Safety’s database in Texas. People possessing a current driver’s licenses and/or an official identification card qualified for selection. Such a process would likely result in a biased sample in that the least acculturated (or recently immigrant) Mexican-descent individuals were likely excluded. Finally, studies that do use representative samples to . investigate Mexican-origin individuals attitudes toward the police frequently rely on 9’ “ unidirnensional ethnic descriptors, such as “Hispanic, new immigrants,” or “Latinos” (Skogan et al., 2002; Tyler, 1990). Doing so fails to differentiate between Latino subgroups, thus potentially masking evidence of differential levels of acculturation. 12 These are a few of the shortcomings inherent in existing studies that the present study seeks to overcome. Tyler (1990) and Skogan and his associates (2002) reported that Hispanics and Latinos were more apathetic toward police officials than Anglo-Americans but less apathetic than African-American individuals. Contact with police and fairness of treatment are variables that are known to affect attitudes toward police among Latinos (Langan, Greenfield, Smith, Durose, & Levin, 2001). Dunham and Alpert (1988), in a study that investigates the influence of neighborhood culture and attitudes toward police in Dade County, find that the less acculturated Cuban-descent individuals are less concerned with police profiling, are more distrustful and wary about police discretion and autonomy, and are more apathetic toward active police strategies than their more acculturated Cuban-descent counterparts. Acculturation and cultural distance affected attitudes toward police strategies among Latinos. Does the concept of acculturation equally apply well to Mexican-descent individuals? Attitudes Toward Youth Deviance Mexican families are characterized by a strong sense of interdependence and collectivism within their extended and nuclear familial relationships (Keefe & Padilla,- 1987; Marin & Marin, 1991; Sabogal, Marin, & Otero-Sabogal, 1987). Caucasian Americans, on the other hand, more strongly value independence and individualism (Gordan, 1964; Vigil, 1998). What is more, at least in comparative terms, traditional Mexican parents tend to be overprotective and highly suspect of aversive extra-familial Social control mechanisms aimed at their children (Horowitz, 1983; Vigil, 1997). When Compared to Anglo-Americans, Mexican parents are also more authoritarian, which 13 sometimes results in stricter discipline and less child autonomy (Zapata & J aramillo, 1981). Sampson & J eglum-Bartusch (1999) found that Latinos were more intolerant of thirteen-year-old deviance when compared to Caucasians. However, the authors did not investigate potential intra-cultural among Latinos. As Mexican parents or adults acculturate and become more “Americanized,” they become less consistent in their use of discipline, less effective at monitoring their children’s whereabouts and activities, which can make their children more vulnerable to deviant peer group influences (Zapata & Jaramillo, 1981). Research supports the link between delinquent behavior and low parental control and regulation (Patterson & Stouthamer-Loeber, 1984; Snyder & Patterson, 1987). The author is unaware, however, of existing research that has investigates the relationship between adult social control practices and acculturation among Mexican-heritage populations. The proposed research project will examine whether more acculturated individuals of Mexican-origin express more lax and tolerant attitudes toward juvenile deviance than their less acculturated counterparts. Attitudes Toward Family Violence Familismo, considered one of the most important culture-specific values in Mexican culture (Alvirez & Bean, 1976), promotes and encourages individuals to strongly identify with and attach themselves to their nuclear and extended families (Marin & Marin, 1991). Familismo is composed of three dimensions: Perceived obligations to provide material and emotional support to the members of the extended family, reliance on relatives for help and support, and the perception of relatives as attitudinal referents (Sabogal, Marin, & Otero-Sabogal, 1987). It is important to 14 distinguish between attitudinal and behavioral components of familismo (also called familialism and farnilism). Attitudinal aspects of familismo refer to beliefs and attitudes that Mexican-descent individuals uphold, wherein strong feelings of loyalty, reciprocity, and solidarity among members of the same family pervade (Sabogal et al., 1987). If the everyday lives of Mexican-oriented individuals lack such bonds, they are more subject to the deleterious effects of acculturative stresses. For example, there is substantial evidence that mothers who mistreat their children perceive their kinship network to be less supportive than do non-abusive mothers (Coohey, 1996). Behavioral familismo, on the other hand, refers to behaviors associated with feelings of familial interdependence (i.e., visiting with or the exchange of favors between family members). In the proposed study, the author will investigate attitudinal familismo. Supporting the hypotheses regarding attitudes toward police, a Mexican national study based on a random sample found that only 20% of Mexican citizens called upon the assistance of public authorities to resolve and mitigate violence within the home (Editor, 1996). Eleven percent of all respondents reported police corruption and inefficiency (among other reasons) as representing obstacles against quelling and mitigating family violence. Paradoxically, nine out of ten Mexican citizens agreed that public protection and regulation was still necessary to resolve and mitigate family violence. In order of preference, respondents invited legal and physical protection of well being from the prosecutor’s office (23%), the commission on human rights (17%), DIF (equivalent to the US FBI) (15%), city police departments (13%), police patrol officers (9 %), state police officers (9 %), and family court judges (7%). Further, 68% of all respondents agreed that victims of family violence must learn to seek help from public authorities. Clearly, this 15 study reveals that there is a significant difference between the rejection of public intervention (when family is experienced) and acceptance of police authority (irregardless of individual experience with family violence). Attitudes and behavior strongly differ in the Mexican context. Similarly, the current study realizes that attitudes toward family violence are not indicative of actual behavior. However, it is expected that Mexican oriented respondents will more likely report family violence as a private concern than their more acculturated counterparts. Further, 92% of the respondents in Tijuana, Mexico reported that children were the worst off victims who suffered in violent households, whereas only 70% responded similarly in Oaxaca, Mexico, one of the southern most cities in Mexico. Family violence is more likely to be reported by more acculturated and urbanized Mexicans living in Tijuana. F arty-six percent of the respondents in Guadalajara, while 35% of the respondents in Morelia, Mexico reported that mothers were the family members who were most frequently physically abused in the household. Generally, violence against women was more likely to be reported and identified in Guadalajara, which is a city located north of the Morelia, Mexico, and is more urbanized and more Americanized. In line with occurrences in Mexico, increased acculturation is reported to have a significant effect on the way “Latino” families seek help or assistance for troubled family members in the United States (Chun & Akutsu, 1999). Among Mexican-Americans, higher levels of acculturation are associated with an increase of help-seeking efforts from extended family members (Keefe, Padilla, & Carlos, 1979; Rogler & Cooney, 1991; Marin & Marin, 1991). Put simply, more acculturated individuals are more likely to seek help outside of their immediate familial network. 16 . :i EDD—Ly I. .. V... ...,_.... ._.J Criminologists have yet to study the insular effects that (attitudinal) familism has on family violence and its commensurability with help seeking efforts and acceptance of outside formal intervention. Most criminological research examines “Latino” attitudes toward substance abuse treatment and domestic violence interventions (Firestone et al., 1999; Torres, 1991; West, Kantor, & J asinski, 1998). Studies on substance abusing “Latinas” (Shearer, Myer, & Ogan, 2001) and “Latinos” (Laureano & Poliandro, 1991) conclude that language barriers and cultural values contribute to their high rates of distrust and cynicism toward formal interventions. However, no theoretical explanations were proffered to explain such differences. In the domestic violence literature, West and associates (1998) found that low acculturation levels, measured by preference for the Spanish language, among “Latinas” are related to their lack of help seeking efforts. This research supports Baker, Perilla, and Norris’s (2001) contention that research on family/domestic violence has been disproportionately focused on female respondents. The use of a unidirnensional measurement of acculturation impinges on the validity of acculturation research. Clearly, not unlike the reasons why “Hispanic” and “Latino” individuals distrust police agencies (Carter, 1985; Safford, 1977) and criminal justice formal interventions (Laureano et al., 1994; Shearer et al., 2001; West et al., 1998), observers from other academic spheres have found that the least acculturated “Latinos” are relatively less willing to access formal sources of help, such as mental and health care resources in the United States (Altarriba & Santiago-Rivera, 1994; Horwitz, 1987). Kanuha (1994) proposed that more acculturated, second- and third-generation Latinos may be more . familiar with and accepting of mental health services (and criminal justice interventions), 17 and thus more likely to seek assistance. This study investigates the effects of acculturation on attitudes toward family violence and acceptance of formal and informal interventions beyond the nuclear family among Mexican-descent men and women. The author hypothesizes that as acculturation rates increase, family violence will be increasingly perceived as a public concern. RESEARCH OBJECTIVE The primary objective of the proposed research is to investigate the differential effects that acculturation has on attitudes toward police officials, youth deviance, and family violence. Accordingly, attention will focus on intra-cultural attitudinal differences among Mexican-descent individuals. For example, differential attitudes toward crime and justice will be assessed between Mexicans, Mexican-Americans, and Chicanos. Finally, the author will conduct a robust test of the effects of acculturation on attitudes toward crime and justice among Mexican-origin individuals by assessing the predictive validity of a multi-item acculturation variable consisting of language use and ethnic identity in a multivariate context. RESEARCH SETTING Because of Chicago’s rich cultural, ethnic, and racial diversity (i.e., Blacks, Whites, and Latinos each represent one-third of the population), it is characteristic of other locales in the United States experiencing surges of pluralism and multiculturalism. Having arrived in the city during World War I, Mexican-Americans are considered Chicago’s first group of Spanish-speaking people (Padilla, 1985). In 1990, 90.5 percent 18 of the Mexican population in the United States was urban (Gonzales, 2000). Echoing the 2000 Census Bureau’s national data on Mexican-descent individuals, Mexicans and Chicanos in Chicago are concentrated in “low-skills” occupations, with roughly half holding jobs as industrial operatives, fabricators, and other types of manual laborers (De Genova, 2003). Mexican-origin individuals represent 60% of the national “Latino” population (U .S. Bureau of Census, 2001). In Chicago, individuals of Mexican-heritage form part of 64% of the “Latino” population (Perlmann & Waldinger, 2000). About half of the Mexican-descent population of Chicago is foreign born (Michelson, 2001; US. Bureau of the Census, 1991). In 2000, the city’s “Latino” population (754,000) exceeded the total population of all but 13 American cities (Skogan, Steiner, DuBois, Gudell, & Fagan, 2002, p. 1). With regards to age, education, and income, the Mexican-descent population of Chicago is very representative of the Mexican-descent population in the entire United States (Michelson, 2001; US. Bureau of the Census, 1991). Because of the intra-cultural diversity of Mexican-oriented Chicagoans, the city provides an ideal research setting for testing hypotheses derived from acculturation theory. METHODS Data This project uses survey data collected in 1995 from the “The Project on Human Development in Chicago Neighborhoods.” The in-person survey was administered to 8,782 Chicago residents. The methodology employed to administer the survey consisted of three steps: first, city blocks within each of the 343 “neighborhood clusters” were selected. Next, residential dwelling from each block were chosen. Finally, one adult 19 resident (i.e., aged 18 or older) for each sampled residence was interviewed (Earls, 1997). Overall, a final response rate of 75% was achieved. These data are representative of Chicago residents. Overall, 2,054 “Latinos” were identified in the survey data where 1,303 of the participants identified themselves as Mexican-descent. Due to missing data, the final analysis file in the current study included 1,134 respondents of Mexican-descent. Dependent Variables The first dependent variable, attitudes toward police, is operationalized as an additive scale using five survey items. Survey respondents were asked how much they agreed or disagreed with the following statements: “The police in this neighborhood are responsive to local issues”; “The police are doing a good job in dealing with problems that really concern people in this neighborhood”; ”The police are not doing a good job in preventing crime in this neighborhood” (reverse coded); “The police do a good job in responding to people in the neighborhood after they have been victims of crime”; and “The police are not able to maintain order on the streets and sidewalks in the neighborhood” (reverse coded). Responses to each of these survey items ranged from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree) (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.74; Mean inter-item correlation = 0.36). Higher scores reflect more positive attitudes toward the police. The second dependent variable, attitudes toward youth deviance, is also operationalized as an additive scale, which uses survey questions regarding youth deviance (i.e., children 13 years of age). Specifically, respondents were asked “how wrong” is it for children to smoke cigarettes, use marijuana, drink alcohol, and get into fist fights? Responses ranged from 1 (not wrong at all) to 5 (extremely wrong) 20 (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.86; Mean inter-item correlation = 0.64). Higher scores reflect less tolerant attitudes regarding to youth deviance. The third outcome variable, attitudes toward family violence, is operationalized using a single survey item. Respondents were asked whether fights between “fi'iends or within families is nobody’s business.” Responses were arranged on a five-item Likert- scale where 1 = strongly disagree and 5 = strongly agree. Higher scores reflect the attitude that violence between family members and friends is a private matter. Descriptive statistics for the variables used in the analyses are provided in Table 1. Table 1. Descriptive Statistics for the Study Variable (N = 1,134) Range Variables Mean SD. MIN MAX Acculturation Items Language 1.74 0.64 1.00 3 .00 Ethnic Identity 1.31 0.52 1.00 3.00 Sociodemographics Education 1 .74 0.99 0.00 5 .00 Sex (1 = male) 0.46 0.50 0.00 1.00 Age (in years) 35.80 12.89 17.00 86.00 Married (1 = married) 0.57 0.50 0.00 1.00 Own Home (l = yes) 0.43 0.50 0.00 1.00 Residential Moves l .00 1 .25 0.00 10.00 Years in Neighborhood 7.00 8.33 0.00 73.00 Control Variable Neighborhood Satisfaction 2.97 0.72 1.00 4.00 Dependent Variables Attitudes Toward Police 16.16 3.68 5.00 25.00 Attitudes Toward Youth Deviance 18.46 2.33 8.00 20.00 Attitudes Toward Family Violence 2.70 1.10 1.00 5 .00 21 Independent Variables The primary independent variable of interest reflects acculturation. As shown n Table 1, acculturation is measured using two factors: ethnic identity (1 = Mexican, 2 = Mexican-American, and 3 = Chicano) and language use at home (1 = primarily Spanish, 2 = both Spanish and English, and 3 = primarily English). In the analyses that follow, the independent effects of each of these indicators of acculturation will be assessed separately, and the combined effect on the dependent variables will also be investigated in a multivariate context. To carry out the latter, it was necessary to construct a composite measure of acculturation. Accomplishing this task involved entering each of the acculturation indicators into a principal components analysis to create a weighted factor score. The results, which are provided in Table 2, show that the two indicators of acculturation (i.e., ethnic identity and language) have identical factors loadings (0.86), which suggests that each item contributes to the latent construct to a similar degree. The results show also that the composite measure captures approximately 73% of the variance associated with acculturation. The values for the acculturation composite scale range from -—1.03 to 3.06. It should be noted that the variable is standardized, so the mean is 0 and the standard deviation equals 1. The bivariate correlation between ethnic identity and language is 0.46 (p g .01). 22 Table 2. Principal Components Analysis Item Factor Loading Ethnic Identity 0.86 Language ' 0.86 Eigenvalue l .46 % of Total Variance Explained 73.04 Bivariate Correlation Coefficient 0.46 Range -1.03 to 3.06 Following the lead of previous assessment of these data (Sampson & J eglum- Bartusch, 1998), a number of demographic variables will also be included. Education is represented as an ordinal-level variable (0 = no school, 1 = less than high school, 2 = high school graduate, 3 = some college, 4 = college graduate, 5 = graduate school). Sex is a dummy variable (1 = male, 0 = female). The effects of marital status will be assessed using a single dummy variable (1 = married, 0 = otherwise). Home ownership reflects whether the respondenthowns their home (1 = yes, 0 = no). Residential moves reflects the number of times the respondent changed residents in the past five years. Finally, age (in years) and years in neighborhood will be continuous variables. One control variable will be included in the multivariate models. Previous assessments of attitudes toward the police have shown that perceptions of neighborhood quality of life influence how individuals view the police (Reisig & Parks, 2000). The control variable neighborhood satisfaction is represented as a five-item additive scale. Respondents were asked, “On the whole, do you like or dislike this neighborhood as a place to live?” Respondents selected from four responses (0 = dislike it a lot, 1 = dislike it, 2 = like it, and 3 = like it a lot). 23 RESULTS Multivariate Analysis of Variance To assess mean differences in the dependent variables between groups exhibiting different levels of acculturation, three multivariate analyses of variance (MANOVA) models were estimated (see Table 3). Previous researchers agree that MAN OVA is conducive to maximizing statistical power to assess differences between groups when using correlated outcome variables (Bray & Maxwell, 1985, pp. 9-11; for an application, see Reisig, 1998, pp'. 237-8). This approach is appropriate for the purposes of the present study. For example, attitudes toward youth deviance is negatively and significantly correlated with attitudes toward family violence (r = -0.64; p 5 0.05). The correlations between the other dependent variables, however, are more modest (see Appendix). The Wilks’ Lambda statistic for all three models reveals that the null hypotheses stating that the population means do not differ can be rejected. In short, these data appear to be appropriate for estimating MAN OVA models. 24 .32 356$ S. w a r... M31v. a r. 5.. w a . ..:8.~ «353 .223 New :m _ .m :woa eacsm a _: OS 25. EN 3.: :2 2.2 E: 2 N 22 EN :2 3.2 S... 2.2 528: 8m 22 a: a2 2.2 on... 8.2 33 nomuau===uu< ten “BBS .223 $06 seemed *mmN ozmflmum k. A2 82 SA SA 2.: 32 E2 :5 imam 4% 2 ._ EN 8d 2.2 NS 8.2 imam Ea Seam 24 2 .2 EN 9% 3.2 2a 8.2 3:0 25% macaw—=5 .33 «uses 2:3 is 2% team 2.2% m cm oi ohm 8a 8.: 8a 3.2 88:6 Nam 22 SN EN 22 SA 8.2 505.5-..852 2w 2._ NZ EN 32 2.... 8.2 :85: 2.53 seem 2 am as: am :82 am as: ooCBOC/ 353m oo=§>ua £u=o> oozom flunk/Orr mufiaflu< @830? mova$~< 6.330% mofi3m3< flute—2 <>OZ<2 .m 039—. 25 Ethnic Identity. Each independent variable (i.e., ethnic identity, language, and acculturation) was aggregated into categories indicating degrees of acculturation. In the first MANOVA model in Table 3, mean values for each of the dependent variables are assessed across three ethnic descriptors - - Mexican, Mexican-American, and Chicano - - that reflect ethnic identity. The results for ethnic identity show that statistically significant differences were observed between groups for attitudes toward police (F = 3.04, p 5 0.05) and youth deviance (F = 2.48, p 5 0.10). Significant differences across ethnic identities were not found for attitudes toward family violence. Results from the Bonferroni multiple comparison tests (not shown) revealed respondents who identified themselves as Mexican had significantly less positive attitudes toward the police (mean = 15.99) when compared to Mexican-Americans (mean = 16.60). Interestingly, the mean score for Chicanos fell between the other two groups. Bonferroni tests also detected significant mean differences between Mexican (mean = 18.52) and Chicanos (mean = 17.60) with respect to attitudes toward youth deviance. Overall, the evidence partially confirms the hypotheses linking acculturation to differential attitudes regard the police and youth deviance. No evidence, however, was revealed to support the contention that individuals expressing more acculturated ethnic identities express different attitudes toward family violence. Language. Moving on to the second MANOVA model in Table 3, language, mean comparisons are once again observed across three distinct groups: Spanish only, Spanish and English, and English only. Once again, significant group differences were observed with regards to attitudes toward police (F = 2.33, p 5 0.05) and attitudes toward youth deviance (F = 4.62, p g 0.01). Similar to the analysis for ethnic identity, however, 26 no significant differences were observed across groups for attitudes toward family violence. Concerning attitudes toward police, the Bonferroni multiple comparison test (not shown) revealed that Mexican-descent respondents who spoke Spanish and English (mean = 16.00) had significantly more apathetic attitudes than their monolingual English speaking counterparts (mean = 16.77). Another interesting finding is that the mean for Spanish speaking Mexican respondents was situated between the other two groups. With regards to attitudes toward youth deviance, Bonferroni tests also showed that there were significant mean differences between individuals who spoke Spanish only (mean = 18.45), Spanish and English (mean = 18.58), and English only (mean = 17.89). The hypotheses addressing attitudes toward police and youth deviance are partially and fully supported, respectively. The Bonferroni tests revealed no significant mean differences across the three linguistic groups as regards attitudes toward family violence. Acculturation. Finally, the last model in Table 3, assesses mean differences in acculturation (i.e., groups formed using an additive variable using ethnic identity and language). To carry out this assessment, respondents were placed into one of three groups reflecting different levels of acculturation: Low, medium, and high. More specifically, those in the “low” acculturation group had scores of either “two” or “three” on the summated acculturation scale, the “medium” group members had a score of four, and those in the “high” acculturation group had scores of “five” or “six.” Because each of the acculturation items had similar factor loading, a simple additive scale was used to group respondents to reflect different overall levels of acculturation for this analysis. Consistent with the findings reported above, statistically significant group differences were observed for attitudes toward police (F = 3.08, p 5 0.05) and attitudes youth 27 deviance (F = 3.13, p 5 0.05). And, once again, no significant differences were observed between groups with regards to attitudes toward family violence. The Bonferroni multiple comparison tests (not shown) revealed significant differences between the least acculturated group (mean = 16.00) and those in the high acculturation group (mean = 16.86) concerning attitudes toward the police. Put simply, more acculturated respondents, on average, reported more favorable attitudes toward the police. Results from the Bonferroni tests also showed that there are significant differences between the “low” group (mean = 18.52) and the “high” group (mean = 17.94) with respect to attitudes toward youth deviance. The acculturation hypotheses were supported: highly acculturated Mexican-oriented individuals favored police and were more tolerant of youth deviance. Again, the Bonferroni tests yielded no significant mean differences with regards to attitudes toward family violence. Ordinary Least-Squares Regression Analysis Ordinary least-squares regression analysis was used to assess the effects of acculturation on all three dependent variables. The dependent variables - - attitudes toward police, attitudes toward family violence, and attitudes toward youth deviance - - were regressed on acculturation and sociodemographic variables (and the statistical control variable in the attitudes toward police model). Because high inter-correlations between independent variables can bias regression estimates, it was necessary to check for potential collinearity. As noted previously, a correlation matrix for the independent variables used to estimate the regression models featured in Table 4 are provided in the Appendix. The bivariate correlation coefficients do not exceed an absolute value of .70, which suggests that collinearity is not cause for concern in the regression analysis. 28 .83 8.5.28 8... v a t... .8... v a t a. .o v a .. 85.0508 5.82%: av 3232652.... 98 .3 322.6538: 2a moogam ”mg-OZ 8... 8... 8... N... anomN cocoa-c neecad eta-7K 8...- 8...- .88 8...- 2..- 8...- .88 8...- ...S. 8... ....8 2... 8:23.32 i- i. :38... a... .28 8.. 8:588 8888.02 8...- S...- ..o.8 8...- t8.- 8...- .88 8...- 2.8.2 ...o. .88 8...- 82.88.02 sea; :3. 8... .88 8... .8..- 8...- .88 2.... 2....- 88 .88 8.8 382.238....3. S...- 88 .88 8...- .2 8... .28 8... 8... 8... .28 2... .8.u:ueomaao 8..- ....8 .88 8...- .8. 8... .38 8... 8...- 8...- .88 3...- Queue-1.83:8 .8..- 8...- .88 8...- :88 8... .88 8... :NZ 8... .88 8... ow< 2... S... .88 2.. 53....- 2.... .38 8...- ..8. 8... ..~.8 8... .22.":me 8...- 8...- .88 8...- 8...- 88 .88 8...- 8... 8... .28 z... 8:88.”. 2.3.8.2 .28 8a .58.... .88 2.2 :........2 i- .88 a... 5388 8.2-. ._ ..m.8 A. use... a ..m.8 a 3.2.. a ..m.8 a 8.8.a> Qua—22> beam 023309 €50> Dom—Om CEBOF "Sweats tasks-H moUBE< 63361—- m0fi3€< 29:22. near-Buoy— mqo .v 03....- 29 Attitudes Toward Police. Five of the nine independent variables were significantly related to attitudes toward police. The primary theoretical variable of interest, acculturation, was positively associated with attitudes toward police ([3 = 0.05; t-value = 1.64). Put simply, more acculturated Mexican-origin respondents expressed more favorable attitudes toward the police. This finding supports the acculturation hypothesis. To explore the dynamics of this finding further, an additional regression model was estimated featuring the two acculturation items (i.e., ethnic identity and language) in a disaggregated format (not shown). The results indicate that the acculturation effect shown in Table 4 is driven by the association with ethnic identity. Put differently, ethnic identity was significantly related to attitudes toward the police (p _<_ 0.05). Language, on the other hand, had a null effect on attitudes toward police. Other significant relationships were also observed: older individuals of Mexican decent (B = 0.07; t-value = 2.12) and male respondents ([3 = 0.05; t-value = 1.68) expressed more positive attitudes toward the police when compared to younger and female respondents. Not surprisingly, neighborhood satisfaction was positively linked to attitudes toward the police ([3 = 0.24; t-value = 8.32). As levels of satisfaction with neighborhood conditions increased among respondents, perceptions of the police became increasingly positive. Finally, respondents who had resided in their neighborhoods for longer periods of time expressed more negative views of the police ([3 = -0.11; t-value = -2.98). It should be noted that these latter four findings are consistent with previous studies of attitudes toward the police using multi-racial and multi-ethnic samples (see Reisig & Parks, 2000; Sampson & J eglum-Bartusch, 1998). The coefficient of 30 determination (R2) reveals that this model explained approximately 7% of the variance associated with attitudes toward the police. Attitudes Toward Youth Deviance. Five of the eight independent variables were significantly related to attitudes toward youth deviance. Although the direction of the regression coefficient is in the hypothesized direction, acculturation was not significantly related to attitudes toward youth deviance ([3 = -0.04; t-value = -1.17, p = .24). In short, the results from the regression model in Table 4 do not support the hypothesis that as Mexican-origin individuals become more acculturated, they are more tolerant of youth _ deviance. A number of statistically significant inverse relationships were observed. In particular, older individuals ([3 = -O.15; t-value = -5.01), those who moved recently ([3 = -0.06; t-value = -1.92), males ([3 = -O.15; t-value = -5.01), and respondents who had lived in their current neighborhood for longer periods, all expressed significantly higher levels of tolerance regarding deviance among thirteen-year-olds. When compared to the attitudes toward police regression model, this model explained less variance (i.e., 4%). Attitudes Toward Family Violence. The slope coefficient for acculturation in the attitudes toward family violence model was not statistically significant (p = .33), thus the hypothesized effect of acculturation was not supported. Nevertheless, the relationship was in the hypothesized direction. The lack of statistical significance may be due, at least in part, to the wording of the survey item. This issue will be revisited in the discussion section. Two of the eight independent variables were significantly related to attitudes toward family violence. As observed in the previous model, age was a statistically significant correlate (B = -0.06; t-value = -1.65). Older individuals of Mexican decent 31 tended to view family violence as more of a public matter. Those who have made more residential moves, on the other hand, were more likely to view family violence as more of a private issue ([3 = 0.07; t—value = 1.95). When compared to the other regression models featured in Table 4, the attitudes toward family violence model explained the least amount of variance (i.e., 2%). DISCUSSION The primary objective of the current research was to dispel the myth of cultural homogeneity by examining the effects of acculturation on attitudes toward police, youth deviance, and family violence among Mexican-origin individuals. Evidence was found in support of acculturation theOry. The most significant findings revealed that Mexican individuals were more apathetic toward police officials than their Mexican-American and Chicano counterparts. Although not significantly significant, Mexicans were more intolerant of youth deviance than their more acculturated counterparts. No evidence was uncovered to suggest that more acculturated Mexican-heritage individuals viewed family violence as a public concem. A more sensitive, multidimensional, and sophisticated acculturation measure could have improved the current analyses and yielded stronger support for the study’s hypotheses. In addition to language and ethnic identity, religious identification could also be included in an acculturation measure.3 Studies show that first generation Mexican-oriented individuals have higher rates of Catholic affiliation when compared to 3 The researcher’s original intent was to include religious identification as a third indicator of acculturation. However, because of time constrictions and delayed notice that the information must be requested with special permission from the distributor, the author decided to proceed with the two most important indicators of acculturation. Nonetheless, religious identification would have accounted for the least amount of variance in the acculturation composite score. 32 subsequent generations (Gonzales & La Velle, 1985; de la Garza, DeSipio, Garcia, Garcia, & Falcon, 1992). Another option would be to use the Acculturation Rating Scale for Mexican-Americans (ARSMA-II) (Cuellar et al., 1995). Not only does acculturation merit more precise measurement in future studies, such tools also must be complimented with survey questions that are also culturally sensitive. For instance, in the present study, acculturation had a null effect on attitudes toward family violence among Mexican-origin respondents. This insignificant effect is more than likely due to the wording of the survey question that asks about violence between “friends” and “family.” The introduction of the subject of friends could have attenuated the effect of familismo among respondents. Such an occurrence would not be surprising since existing studies on familismo do not mention the relevance of friendship networks (Marin & Marin, 1991; Sabogal et al., 1987). Thus, it is beneficial that future criminologists reflect their plight for cultural relativism equally in their measures. Moreover, informed by inquiries in other social sciences (i.e., cultural and psychological anthropology), the present criminological research has reaped the benefits of inter- disciplinary enterprises that can effectively enlighten police practices. In this research, ethnic identity was composed of more complex properties than previous research had reported (e. g., Buriel & Cardoz, 1993; Phinney & Devich—Navarro, 1997). For example, each of the three ethnic labels affected attitudes differently among Mexican-oriented respondents. Specifically, the Chicano social identity differed in important aspects that merit closer inspection. First, only 30 respondents identified themselves as “Chicano,” whereas 292 and 812 respondents subscribed to “Mexican- American” and “Mexican” social identities, respectively. This supports Vigil’s (1997) 33 observation regarding the decline of the subscription to Chicano ethnic identity. Further, the Chicano ethnic label in the 19605, 19705, and early 19805 represented a solid and strong ethnic and cultural identity among Mexican-oriented individuals (Horowitz, 1983; Penalosa, 1973). As the ethnic category became more politically charged and unattractive, more youth during the early 19905 refrained from identifying with it (Vigil, 1997). However, in the current study, young females and those who had not completed high school more fiequently identified with the Chicana ethnic identity. Supporting previous research (Gurin, Hurtado, & Peng, 1994), these findings reveal that this ethnic identity was more than likely politically-oriented in comparison to the class-oriented Mexican and Mexican-American ethnic identities (measured by educational achievement). Gurin and her colleagues (1994) argue that the subscription to the Chicano/a ethnic identity leads to more inter-group interaction, therefore attenuating the effect of acculturation for this particular ethnic group. Put simply, political involvement and social class, not unlike previous research (see Sampson & J eglum-Bartusch, 1998), attenuates some cultural effects, in this case, acculturation. This study justifies and invites future research that both combines Mexican- American and Chicano individuals as one group and research that differentiates between the two. On the one hand, individuals in this study who identified with the ethnic identities of Mexican and Mexican-Americans differed significantly fi'om each other when viewing police officials, yet Chicanos were situated between the two extremes. However, Chicanos and Mexican-Americans mean scores approximated each other and they could arguably be aggregated together. On the other hand, Chicanos, Mexican- Americans, and Mexicans varied notably with respect to tolerance of youth deviance, 34 siding with the notion of treating all three ethnic groups as unique and distinct. Ethnic identity elucidated important cross cutting features within the construct of acculturation. In the future, qualitative research may help explain such interesting intersections (see Vigil, 1988, 2002). Just like linguistic anthropologists (Duranti, 1994; Kroskrity, 1993; Zentella, 1997) and psychological anthropologists (Fiske, 1991; Ward, 1992), the researcher considered culture, psychology, and language as interrelated phenomena. Although this study suggested the (independent effects of language preference and ethnic identity, the concept of acculturation nonetheless retained its holistic features, thus keeping with the tradition established by cultural, sociological, and psychological scholars regarding inextricably tied phenomena. Chicago was composed mostly of Mexicans (N = 805) who scored low on the acculturation spectrum. Notably, these cross-sectional data provides weak support for the assimilation hypothesis, especially since rates for English monolingualism (N = 125), Chicanoism (N = 30), and assimilation rates (N = 111) are low. Furthermore, Mexican- descent individuals in the United States, according to the data, favor both separatist and integrationist adaptive sub-strategies that Berry (1997) suggests in his theoretical work. Mexicans were reportedly separatists, because of the high rates of Spanish 1 monolingualism (N = 415), Mexican ethnic identities (N = 812), and low acculturation levels among respondents. The integrationist school of thought is less but certainly supported by the high rates of bilingualism (N = 594) among Mexican-descent respondents. Superficially, the everyday life and routine of Mexican-descent immigrants and their offspring requires them to interact fully in both English and Spanish in the name 35 of convenience, not affecting other culturally separatist features (i.e., language preference, acculturation, etc.). One subsequent study has been administered each year for six years to the same respondents in this study. As soon as these data are available, longitudinal analyses will test the acculturation, assimilation, and adaptation directional hypotheses aforementioned. It will fill the void where Berry (1997) neither emphasized nor offered clear examples supporting or disconfirming the case for mutual dependency or mutual exclusivity among these strategies (i.e., integrationist, separatist, and assimilationist). The goal of future cultural criminological studies must comprehend and scrutinize these sub-processes and effectively express how these apply to different cultural groups. For instance, is it that police departments, which are extensions of a larger political infrastructure, segregate these groups, or is it that these cultural groups separate themselves altogether from police entities due to the their discriminatory and profiling practices? How does acculturation influence these rates? Political studies show that Mexican-Americans are less trustful of American governments than their less acculturated co-ethnic counterparts (Klesner, 2003; Michelson, 2001a, 2001b). The current study, then, strongly questions the police departments’ instrumental and auxiliary attachment to the greater governmental infrastructure, because Mexicans in this study firmly viewed police less favorably than their more acculturated counterparts. Ferrel (1999), in his description of “cultural criminology” fails to realize the importance of representative, quantitative, and probabilistic studies, especially since he emphasizes importance of qualitative studies. Ironically, he fails to recognize in the research that he proposes the existing multiplicity of American cultures in a growingly 36 multicultural criminal justice setting. This study introduces new forms of inquiry to the school of “cultural criminology.” Specifically this study gives import to probabilistic, large, representative, and quantitative research schemas for studying cultural phenomena within unique American cultural groups. Thus, equally effective as any good qualitative study, the current quantitative study yielded important findings relevant to cultural studies with regard to attitudes toward police and delinquency especially. Criminological theories and current criminal justice policy making practices have lumped Latin American-origin individuals into one group, which has been categorized as either “Latino” or “Hispanic,” negating important inter- and intra-cultural differences (Garcia, 1994; Pizarro & Vera, 2001). The current research successfully questions the efficacy and saliency of criminal justice policies that lump individuals into aggregates or quantifiable categories deemed manageable and stripped of their cultural sensibilities. For penal policymakers and police agencies to employ one-size-fits-all crime control, intervention, and preventative strategies is to insulate themselves from the “messy, hard- to-control demands of the social world” (F eeley & Simon, 1992, p. 456). In this research, this messiness is caused by the effects of acculturation and other cultural processes. This research succeeded in deconstructing such aggregate modes of thinking. Criminologists and criminal justice practitioners must now work on reconstructing these categories and sub-categories faithfully, which will be conducive to culturally specific and relative policy implementation in the criminal justice system within a multicultural context. 37 APPENDIX 38 .92 3:35;; 8. w m : 3. w a .. 2: :86 1.8.? cad Ed- :aod mod wed 36 need Nod couoflmtum uoofonnwfiz : co; .33.? .2..de vod. {acmd mod shod :mmd :omd 1.3.9 woofenanZEmSo> 2 2: :3? 3.? :3? 8.? 8.? 3.? 8.? 8.? 38238282 a 2: :23 :23 23 ::.o :23 :83 ::.3 escapee w 2: :23 .83 :23 :2? :2? :2? 3252 a. 2: 8.3 :8? 8.3 :3 23 a? e 2: :3 3.? :3? 5.3 6m m 2: :23 :23 :32 8:258 v 2: :33 :33 8:82.82 m 2: :23 3:52 25:: N 2: owazmcmq _ : S a w a e m. e m N _ 329323 3:023:3qu 8.. 2.32 seam—280 26:33:. 39 REFERENCES Adler, F ., Mueller, G.O., & Laufer, W.S. (1991). Criminology. New York: McGraw-Hill. Altarriba, J., & Santiago-Rivera, A.L. (1994). Current perpectives on using linguistic and cultural factors in counseling the Hispanic client. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 25, 388-397. Alvirez, D., & Bean, FD. (1976). The Mexican American family. In C.H. Mindel & R.N. Haberstein (Eds.), Ethnic families in America (pp. 271-291) New York: Elsevier. Aycan, Z., & Berry, J .W. (1996). Impact of employment-related experiences on immigrants’ well-being and adaptation to Canada. Canadian Journal of Behavioral Science, 28, 240-251. Baker, C.K., Perilla, J .L., & Norris, EH. (2001). Parenting stress and parenting competence among Latino men who batter. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 16, 1139- 1157. Bedard, L., Eschholz, S., & Gertz, M. (1994). Perceptions of crime and education within the Hispanic community and the impact on corrections. Journal of Correctional Education, 45, 72-80. Berry, J.W. (1992a). Cross-cultural psychology: Research and applications. New York: Cambridge University Press. Beny. J. W. (1992b). Acculturation and adaptation in a new society. International Migration, 30, 69-85. Berry, J . W. (1997). Immigration, acculturation, and adaptation. Applied Psychology: An International Review, 46, 5-68. Beny, J.W., & Annis, RC. (1974). Acculturative stress. Journal ofCross-Cultural Psychology, 5, 382-406. Berry, J.W., & Sam, D. (1996). Acculturation and adaptation. In J.W. Berry, M.H. Segall, at C. Kagicibasi (Eds), Handbook of cross-cultural psychology, Vol. 3. Boston: Allyn & acon. Botello, N.A., & Rivera, A.L. (2000). Everything in this job is money: Inside the Mexrcan police. World Policy Journal, 17, 61-71. Blea, 1.1. (1988). Toward a Chicano Social Science. New York: Praeger. 40 Bray, J.H., & Maxwell, SE. (1985). Multivariate Analysis of Variance. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Buffmgton, R. (2000). Criminal and Citizen in Modern Mexico. Lincoln, NB: University of Nebraska Press. Buriel, R., Perez, W. De Ment, T.L., Chavez, D.V., & Moran, V.R. (1998). The relationship of language brokering to academic performance, biculturalism, and self- efficacy among Latino adolescents, Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 20, 283- 297. Buriel, R., & Cardoza, D. (1993). Mexican American ethnic labeling: An intrafamilial and intergenerational analysis. In J.P. & M. Rotherman (Eds.), Children ’s ethnic socialization: Pluralism and development (pp. 134-152). Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Caetano, R., Schafer, J ., Clark, C.L., Cunradi, C.B., & Raspberry, K. (2000). Intimate partner violence, acculturation, and alcohol consumption among Hispanic couples in the United States. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 15, 30-45. Carter, D. L. (1983). Hispanic interaction with the criminal justice system in Texas: Experiences, attitudes, and perceptions. Journal of Criminal Justice, 11, 213-227. Carter, UL. (1985). Hispanic perception of police performance: An empirical assessment. Journal of Criminal Justice, 13, 487-500. Chavez, LR. (1998). Shadowed Lives: Undocumented Immigrants in American Society. New York: Harcourt Brace. Cheurprakobkit, S. (2000). Police-citizen contact and police performance: attitudinal differences between Hispanics and non-Hispanics. Journal of Criminal Justice, 28, 325- 336. Chevigny, P. (1999). Defining the role of the police in Latin America. In J.E. Mendez, G. O’Donnell & P. S. Pinheiro (Eds), The (un)rule of law and the underprivileged in Latin America (pp. 49-70). South Bend, IN: University of Notre Dame Press. Chevigny, P. (1995). Edge of the Knife: Police Violence in the Americas. New York: The New Press. Chun, K.M., & Akutsu, PD. (1999). Utilization of mental health services. In E.J. Kramer, S.L. Ivey, & Y.W. Ying (Eds), Immigrant women 's health: Problems and solutions (pp.54-64). San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass. Coohey, C. (1996). Child maltreatment: Testing the social isolation hypothesis. Child Abuse and Neglect, 20, 241-254. 41 Costelloe, M., Chiricos, T., Burianek, J ., Gertz, M., & Maier-Katkin, D. (2002). The , social correlates of punitiveness toward criminals: A comparison of the Czech republic and Florida. Justice System Journal, 23, 191-223. Cuellar, 1., Arnold, B., & Maldonado, R. (1995). Acculturation rating scale for Mexican- Americans: A revision of the original ARSMA scale. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 17, 275-304. De Genova, N. (1998). Race, space, and reinvention of Latin America in Mexican Chicago. Latin American Perspectives, 25, 87-116. de la Garza, R. O., Desipio, L., Garcia, F .C., Garcia, J ., & Falcon, A. (1992). Latino Voices: Mexican, Puerto Rican, and Cuban Perspectives. Boulder, CO: Westview Press. Di Rado, A. (October 25, 1993). The times poll: The fear of crime is the unifying factor in orange county. Los Angeles Times, p. A1. Dunham, R.G., & Alpert, GP. (1988). Neighborhood differences in attitudes toward policing evidence for a mixed strategy model of policing in a multi ethnic setting. Journal of Criminal Law & Criminology, 79, 504-523. Duranti, A. (1994). From Grammar to Politics: Linguistic Anthropology in a Western Samoan Village. Berkeley and Los Angeles: University of California Press. Earls, F. (1999). Project on human development in Chicago neighborhoods: Community survey, [994-] 995 [Computer file]. Ann Arbor, MI: Inter-university Consortium for Political and Social Research. Editor. (1996). Estudio de penetracion y opinion publica sobre la violencia familiar, auspiciado por el fondo de poblacion de las naciones unidas. Nueva Cultura F eminista, 20, 34-39. Feeley, M.M., & Simon, J. (1992) The new penology: Notes on the emerging strategy of corrections and its implications. Criminology, 30, 449-474. Ferrell, J. (1999) Cultural criminology. Annual Review of Sociology, 25, 395-418. Firestone, J.M., Lambert, L.C., & Vega. WA. (1999). Intimate Violence Among Woman of Mexican Origin: Correlates of Abuse. Journal of Gender, Culture, and Health, 4, 199- 234. Fiske, AP. (1996). The cultured mind: Why psyches depend on cultures. Review of Philip K. Bock, Psychological Anthropology. Contemporary Psychology, 41, 929—930. Garcia, R. (1994). Latinos and Criminal Justice. Chicano-Latino Law Review, 14, 6-19. 42 . . n uni ”fir! Garza, H. (1974). Administration of justice: Chicanos in Monterrey County. In A.S. Lopez (Ed.), Criminal justice and Latino communities (pp. 47-55). New York: Garland. Gonzales, M.G. (2000). Mexicanos: A History of Mexicans in the United States. Indianapolis, IN: Indiana University Press. Gonzales, R.O., & La Velle, M. (1985). The Hispanic Catholic in the United States. New York: Northeast Catholic Pastorla Center for Hispanics. Gordon, M. (1964). Assimilation in American Life. New York: Oxford University Press. Gurin, P., Hurtado, A., & Peng, T. (1994) Group contacts and ethnicity in the social identities of Mexicanos and Chicanos. Personality and Social Psychologr Bulletin, 20, 521-532. Horowitz, R. (1983). Honor and the American Dream. New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press. Horwitz, A.V. (1987). Help-seeking processes and mental health services. In D. Mechanic (Ed.) Improving mental health services: What the social sciences can tell us (pp. 33-45). San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass. Johnson, AG, (2000). The Blackwell Dictionary of Sociology: A User ’3 Guide to Sociological Language. Malden, MA: Blackwell. Kanuha, V. (1994). Women of color in baterring relationships. In L. Comas-Diaz & B. Greene (Ed5.), Women of color: Integrating ethnic and gender identities in psychotherapy (pp. 428-454). New York: Guilford Press. Keefe, S. E., & Padilla, A. (1987). Chicano Ethnicity. Albuquerque, NM: University of New Mexico Press. Keefe, S.E., Padilla, A.M., & Carlos, ML. (1979). The Mexican-American extended family as an emotional support system. Human Organization, 34, 144-152. Klesner, J .L. (2003). Political Attitudes, Social Capital, and Political Participation: The United States and Mexico Compared. Mexican Studies, 19, 29-63. Kottack, C. P. (2002). Anthropology: The Exploration of Human Diversity. Boston: McGraw-Hill. Kroskrity, Paul V. (1993). Language, History, and Identity: Ethnolinguistic Studies of the Arizona T ewa. Tucson, AZ: University of Arizona Press. 43 Laureano, M., & Poliandro, E. (1991). Understanding cultural values of Latino male alcoholics and their families: A cultural sensitive model. Journal of Chemical Dependency Treatment, 4,137-155. Lafree, G., & Russell, K.K. (1993). The argument for studying race and crime. Journal of Criminal Justice Education, 4, 273-288. Langan, P.A. Greenfield, L.A., Smith, S.K., Durose, M.R., & Levin, DJ. (2001). Contacts Between the Police and the Public: Findings from the 1999 National Survey. Washington, DC: US. Department of Justice. Levy, K. (2001). Relationships between adolescent attitudes toward self-concept, authority, and delinquency. Adolescence, 36, 333-346. Marin, G., & Marin, B.V. (1991). Research with Hispanic populations. Newbury Park: Sage. ' Marin, G. Sabogal, E, Marin, B.V., Otero-Sabogal, R., & Perez-Stable, E. (1987). Development of a short acculturation scale. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Science, 9, 183-205. Meier, M.S., & Ribera, F. (1993). Mexican Americans/American Mexicans: From Conquistadores to Chicanos. Canada: Harper Collins. Michelson, M.R. (2001a). Political trust among Chicago Latinos. Journal of Urban Affairs, 23, 323-334. Michelson, M.R. (2001b). The effect of national mood on Mexican American political opinion. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 23, 57-70. Mirande, A. (1980). Fear of Crime and Frear of the Police in a Chicano Community. Sociology and Social Research, 64, 528-541. Mirande, A. (1981). The Chicano and the Law: An analysis of community-police conflict in an urban barrio. Pacific Sociological Review, 24, 65-86. Mirande, A., (1987). Gringo Justice. South Bend, IN: University of Notre Dame Press. Myers, L.B., Cintron, M., Kathryn, & Scarborough, E. (1994). Latinos: The conceptualization of race. In J .E. Hendricks & B. Byers (Eds) Multicultural perspectives in criminal justice and criminology (pp 155-184). Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas. Olmeda, EL. (1979). Acculturation: A psychometric perspective. American Psychologist, 34, 1061-1070. 44 Padilla, F .M. (1985). Latino Ethnic Consciousness: The case of Mexican Americans and Puerto Ricans in Chicago. South Bend, IN: University of Notre Dame Press Padilla, F .M. (1986). On the nature of Latino ethnicity. Social Science Quarterly, 65, 65 1-664. Palerm, J .V., Vincent, B.R., & Vincent, K. (1999). Mexican Immigrants in Courts. In J .1. Moore and ME. Fisher (Eds), Immigrants in courts (pp. 73-97). Seattle, WA: University of Washington Press. Patterson, G. R., & Stouthamer-Loeber, M. (1984). The correlation of family management practices and delinquency. Child Development, 55, 1299-1307. Penalosa, F. (1973). Toward an operational definition of the Mexican-American. In G. Lopez & G. Rivas (Eds), The Chicanos (pp. 91-106). New York: Monthly Review Press. Perez-McCluskey, C. (2002). Understanding Latino Delinquency. New York: LFB Scholarly Publishing LLC. Perlrnann, J ., & Waldinger, R. (1998). Are the children of today’s immigrants making it? In S. Steinberg (Ed.), Race and ethnicity in the United States (pp.223-252). Malden, MA: Blackwell. Phinney, J ., & Devich-Navarro, M. (1997). Variations in bicultural identification among African American and Mexican American adolescents. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 7, 3-32. Phinney, J. (1992). The multigroup ethnic identity measure: A new scale for use with diverse groups. Journal of Adolescent Research, 7, 156-176. Pizarro, M., & Vera, EM. (2001) Chicana/o ethnic identity research: Lessons for researchers and counselors. The Counseling Psychologist, 29, 91-117. Ponce, C., & Comer, B. (2003). Is Acculturation in Hispanic Health Research a Flawed Concept? JRSI Working Paper, no. 60. Rebach, H. (1992). Alcohol use among American minorities. In J .S. Trimble, C.S. Bolek, & S.J. Niemcryk (Eds), Ethnic and multicultural drug Abuse: Perspectives on current research. New York: The Haworth Press. Redfield, R., Linton, R., & Herskovitz, M.J. (1936). Memorandum for the study of acculturation, American Anthropologist, 38, 149-152. Reisig, MD. (1998). Rates of disorder in higher-custody state prisons: A comparative analysis of managerial practices. Crime and Delinquency, 44, 229-244. 45 Reisig, M.D., & Parks, RB. (2000). Experience, quality of life, and neighborhood context: A hierarchical analysis of satisfaction with police. Justice Quarterly, 17, 607- 630. Richmond, A. (1993). Reactive migration: Sociological perspectives on refugee movements. Journal of Refugee Studies, 6, 7-24. Rogler, L.H., & Cooney, RS. (1991). Puerto Rican families in New York City: Intergenerational processes. Marriage and Family Review, 16, 331-349. Rosenfeld, M.J. (2001). Measures of assimilation in the marriage market: Mexican Americans 1970-1990. Journal of Marriage and Family, 64, 152-162. Sabogal, E, Marin, B., & Otero-Sabogal, R. (1987). Hispanic familism and acculturation: What changes and what doesn’t. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 9, 397-412. Safford, J.B. (197 7). No comprendo: The non-english defendant and the criminal process. The Journal of Criminal Law & Criminology, 68, 15-30. Samaniego, R.Y., & Gonzales, NA. (1999). Multiple Mediators of the Effects of Acculturation Status on Delinquency for Mexican American Adolescents. American Journal of Community Psychology. 27, 189-210. Sampson, R.J., & J eglum-Bartusch, DJ. (1998). Legal cynicism and (subcultural?) tolerance of deviance: The neighborhood of racial differences. Law & Society Review, 32, 777-804. Schwarzer, R., Hahn, A., & Schroder, H. (1994). Social integration and social support in a life crisis: Effects of macrosocial change in East Germany. American Journal of Community Psychology, 22, 685-706. Shearer, R.A., Myers, L.B., & Ogan, GD. (2001). Treatment resistance and ethnicity among female offenders in substance abuse treatment programs. The Prison Journal, 81, 55-72. Skogan, W.G., Steiner, L., DuBois, J ., Gudell, E.J., & Fagan, J. (2002). Community Policing and “The New Immigrants Latinos in Chicago. Washington, DC: Government Printing Office. Snyder, J ., & Patterson, G. (1987). Family interaction and delinquent behavior. In H.C. Quay (Ed.), Handbook of juvenile delinquency (pp. 216-243). New York: Wiley. Stanfield, J.H. (1993). Epistemological considerations. In J .H. Stanfield & R.M. Dennis (Eds), Race and ethnicity in research methods (pp. 16-36) Newbury Park, CA: Sage. 46 '- Takagi, P. (1981). Race, crime and social policy: A minority perspective. Crime & Delinquency, 27, 48-63. Torres, 8., & Vogel, RE. (2001). Pre- and post-test differences between Vietnamese and Latino residents involved in a community policing experiment: Reducing fear of crime and improving attitudes towards the police. Policing, 24, 40-56. Trujillo, L. D. (1974). “La evolucion del “bandido” al ‘Pachuco’”: A critical examination and evaluation of criminological literature on Chicanos. Issues in Criminology, 9, 43-67. Tyler, TR. (1990). Why People Obey the Law. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. US. Bureau of the Census (2001). Census of Population, 2000. Washington, DC: Government Printing Office. US. Bureau of the Census. (1991). Census of Population, 1990. Washington, DC: Government Printing Office. Vasquez, A. (1984). Les implications ideologiques du concept d’acculturation. Cahiers de Sociologie Economique et Culturelle, 1, 83-121. Vaughn, M.S., Deng, F ., & Lee, L. (2003). Evaluating a drug treatment program in Taiwan. Journal of Drug Issues, 33, 357-367. Vega, W.A., Khoury, E.L., Zimmerman, R.S., Gil, A.G., & Warheit, G.J. (1995). Cultural conflicts and problem behaviors of Latino adolescents in home and school environments. Journal of Community Psychology, 23, 167-179. Vigil, J .D. (2002) A Rainbow of Gangs: Street Cultures in the Megacity. Austin: University of Texas Press. Vigil, J .D. (1997). Personas Mexicanas: Chicano High Schoolers in a Changing Los Angeles. Orlando, FL: Harcourt Brace. Vigil, J .D. (1998). From Indians to Chicanos: The Dynamics of Mexican-American Culture. Prospect Heights, IL: Waveland. Vigil, JD. (1988) Barrio Gangs: Street Life and Identity in Southern California. Austin: University of Texas Press. Wall, J .A., Power, T.G., & Arbona, C. (1993). Susceptibility to Antisocial Peer pressure and Its Relation to Acculturation in Mexican- American Adolescents. Journal of Adolescent Research. 8, 403- 418. Ward, C. (1996). Acculturation. In D. Lands & R.S. Bhagat (Eds), Handbook of intercultural training (pp. 124-147). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. 47 Ward, C., & Kennedy, A. (1992). Locus of control , mood disturbance and social difficulty during cross-cultural transitions. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 16, 175-194. Weitzer, R., & Tuch, SA. (1999). Race, class, and perceptions of discrimination by the police. Crime & Delinquency, 45, 494-507. West, C., Kantor, G.K., & J asinski, J .L. (1998). Sociodemographic predictors and cultural barriers to help-seeking behavior by Latina and Anglo-American battered women. Violence and Victims, 13, 361-375. Zapata, J .T., & J aramillo, PT (1981). Research on the Mexican-American family. Journal of Individual Psychology, 37, 72-85. Zayas, L.H., Rojas, M., & Malgady, R.G. (1998). Alcohol and Drug Use, and Depression Among Hispanic Men in Early Adulthood. American Journal of Community Psychology, 26, 425-438. Zentella, A.C. (1997). Growing Up Bilingual. Malden, MA: Blackwell. Zule,W.A., Desmond, D.P., Medrano, M.A., & Hatch, J .P. (2001). Acculturation and risky injection practices among Hispanic injectors. Evaluation and Program, 24, 207- 214. 48