Em.» ,. .._ .n - I ; v a «an ,o t ‘ 1.1‘3‘! ’ I; nrv“"'" m} ”w”. ’3 LIBRARIES )7) I”? MICHIGAN STATE UNIVERSITY . ,. EAST LANSING, MICI-I 48824-1048 wMO‘é‘Z’ (:7 This is to certify that the dissertation entitled KINDERGARTEN TEACHER PRECEPTIONS REGARDING THE DEVELOPMENT OF YOUNG CHILDREN'S EMERGING LITERACY SKILLS presented by PEGGY ANN THELEN has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for the Ph. D. degree in Family and Child Ecology L. / - "7” \ , L 3‘! I (r W"! Major Professor’s Signature ,JoL/r; /0 ‘7! Date MSU is an Affinnative Action/Equal Opportunity Institution .7 ‘444- ‘ o O -- PLACE IN RETURN BOX to remove this checkout from your record. TO AVOID FINES return on or before date due. MAY BE RECALLED with earlier due date if requested. DATE DUE DATE DUE DATE DUE 35f22$33®8 6/01 c:/ClRC/DateDue.p65-p. 15 KINDERGARTEN TEACHER PERCEPTIONS REGARDING THE DEVELOPMENT OF YOUNG CHILDREN’S EMERGING LITERACY SKILLS By Peggy Ann Thelen A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University In partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Family and Child Ecology 2004 ABSTRACT KINDERGARTEN TEACHER PERCEPTIONS REGARDING THE DEVELOPMENT OF YOUNG CHILDREN’S EMERGING LITERACY SKILLS By Peggy Ann Thelen This study examined kindergarten teacher perceptions regarding the development of young children’s emerging literacy skills. Using data from a cross-sectional design sample of 42 kindergarten teachers, the investigation found that many kindergarten teachers did not have a complete understanding relative to the developmental path of emerging literacy skills. Specifically, there seemed to be a lack of awareness as to when certain foundational emerging literacy skills should be developed, and the impact those skills have on the development of future, more abstract skills. A majority of teachers in this study indicated emerging literacy skills such as recognize rhyming words, understand the difference between pictures and print, and understand that letters are formed in a specific way and are unchanging should be developed alter kindergarten entry. Whereas, these teachers also indicated that the more abstract skills write some letters of the alphabet, wn'te first name, and recognize own name in writing should be developed prior to kindergarten entrance. Differences in teacher perspectives emerged relating to several personal characteristics including the number of years of teaching experience, preschool teaching experience, early childhood training, and teacher participation in professional activities, as well as contextual characteristics including the type of program the teacher taught in (Le. half-day or all day program), classroom size, and children’s prior program experience. Teachers with less teaching experience, those with a great deal of preschool teaching experience, teachers with less/no early childhood training, and those who did not participate in professional activities had higher expectations for the development of certain emerging literacy skills than their counterparts. Teachers in all day, every day kindergarten programs, and those with smaller class sizes, expected certain emerging literacy skills to be developed earlier than teachers in half-day programs or those with more students. The salutary effects of comparing expectations of these professionals involved in children’s literacy development include implications for preservice and inservice training in the field of education. Copyright by PEGGY A. THELEN 2004 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS This work is the result of much encouragement and support from many people. I am most grateful for the faith that my family, friends, and the faculty at Michigan State University had that this body of research would be accomplished. Many exceptional people have played a key role in my academic and professional development, in particular my committee members and several professors in the Family and Child Ecology Deparb'nent. My advisor and mentor, Dr. Anne Soderrnan, has played an exceptionally strong part in my growth. She continually offered opportunities to challenge my abilities and increase my knowledge in the areas of child development, early childhood education, and most particularly emerging literacy. Her guidance and faith in me have been invaluable. Dr. Alice Whiren has been a tremendous support and a valuable resource of knowledge. I am forever grateful for her strong encouragement to participate in professional activities, whether I was ready or not. Dr. Robert Gn’ffore has been a tremendous help in guiding me through the statistical process and helping me to realize it does not have to be terrifying. Dr. Nell Duke offered many important suggestions in making this a stronger research study. Her advice and insight have been indispensable. Significant support for this study has also come from the Ingham County Intermediate School Distict. Working closely with Michelle Nicholson and her staff made the initiation and implementation of this study achievable. Other professionals have offered much appreciated support. Christie Freestone and the kindergarten staff at Ithaca South Elementary, and Donnalyn lngersol and the kindergarten staff at Luce Road Elementary participated in the pilot study. Linda Chard extended help in keeping the statistical process on track. Leslie Thomas lent her expertise in language and continuity. Their collaboration and kindness through these years has been valued beyond words. Exceptional motivators and supporters include my extended family and friends. They kept me looking forward and staying focused. Above all, my children, Caitlin and Kelsey, and my husband, Teny, deserve the most recognition for their help and encouragement. Their willingness to help out wherever needed has been a major reason for this accomplishment. I am truly appreciative of their love and support. vi TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES ....................................................................................... x LIST OF FIGURES .................................................................................... xii CHAPTER ONE: INTRODUCTION ........................................................... 1 Need for the Study ........................................................................... 3 Purpose of the Study ........................................................................ 5 Statement of the Problem ................................................................. 5 Conceptual Framework .................................................................... 7 Ecological Systems Theory .................................................. 7 Process-Person-Context Model ............................................ 9 Conceptual and Operational Definitions ......................................... 12 Research Questions ....................................................................... 14 Assumptions ................................................................................... 14 Summary ........................................................................................ 15 CHAPTER 'IWO: REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE ................................ 16 Children’s Emergent Literacy ......................................................... 16 Parents As Teachers ...................................................................... 29 Sociocultural Effects on the Development of Emerging Literacy .......................................................................... 36 Readiness Education Prior to Kindergarten Entry .......................... 46 Childcare ............................................................................ 51 Preschool ........................................................................... 53 Characteristies of Early Childhood Programs and Teachers ........................................................................................ 56 Kindergarten Eduation: Yesterday and Today ............................. 61 Summary ........................................................................................ 73 CHAPTER THREE: METHODS ............................................................. 76 Research Design ............................................................................ 76 Variables ........................................................................................ 76 Subjects ......................................................................................... 76 Instrumentation ............................................................................... 78 Data Collection ............................................................................... 84 Analyses ......................................................................................... 85 Limitations ...................................................................................... 91 Summary ........................................................................................ 92 CHAPTER FOUR: RESULTS ................................................................. 93 Demographic Characteristics of the Sample .................................. 94 vii Question One: Which specific emerging literacy skills do kindergarten teachers believe they are primarily responsible for teaching? 98 Question Two: Is there any agreement among kindergarten teachers as to which emerging literacy skills they believe are best acquired prior to kindergarten entrance? .................................. 101 Question Three: Is there any agreement among kindergarten teachers as to which emerging literacy skills they believe are best acquired after kindergarten entrance? ...................................... 103 Question Four. Is there any agreement among kindergarten teachers as to vrmich emerging literacy skills they believe are best acquired at the beginning of first grade? ................................. 105 Question Five: Do Perceptions of the acquisition of emerging literacy skills vary as a function of the demographies of kindergarten teachers? .............................................................. 106 Question Six: What sources influence kindergarten teachers’ expectations of children’s literacy readiness? ........... 127 Question Seven: Are kindergarten teachers’ beliefs congruent with others regarding children’s literacy capabilities? .............................................................................. 131 Question Eight: Which emerging literacy skills do kindergarten teachers believe are the five most essential skills children should develop by kindergarten entry? ............... 134 Question Nine: Are kindergarten teacher expectations of skills to be developed during the kindergarten year congruent with the State of Michigan Kindergarten Framework of Skills? ................................................................ 137 Additional Findings ...................................................................... 139 Summary .................................................................................... 140 viii CHAPTER FIVE: DISCUSSION ............................................................ 141 Foundational Skill Recognition .................................................... 142 Phonological Awareness .................................................. 143 Concepts of Print .............................................................. 143 Alphabet ........................................................................... 144 Patterns of Expectations ............................................................. 145 Teaching Background ...................................................... 145 Education Background ..................................................... 146 Classer Characteristies ................................................ 148 Additional Factors Investigated for Teacher Perspectives ................................................................................ 150 Influences on Teacher’s Expectations .............................. 150 Congruency of Belief Regarding Children’s Literacy Capabilities ....................................................................... 151 Congruency of Beliefs With Local School District ............. 152 Michigan Curriculum Framework of Expectations ............ 153 Ecological Perspectives .................................................... 153 Implications for Classroom Practice ............................................ 154 Implications for ln-service and Preservice Education .................. 155 Implications for Future Research ................................................. 157 Summary ..................................................................................... 159 APPENDICES ........................................................................................ 161 Appendix A: Michigan Curriculum Framework Kindergarten Expectations ................................... 163 Appendix B: Emergent Literacy Skills Survey ............................ 169 Appendix C: Letter of Teacher Consent ..................................... 175 Appendix D: Principal Letter ....................................................... 177 Appendix E: University Letter of Consent for Research ............. 179 REFERENCES ....................................................................................... 181 10. 11. 12. 13. LIST OF TABLES Approximate Historical Timeframe ................................................ 48 lnstnrment Item Literacy Categories and Item Source .................. 82 Summary of Research Questions, Hypotheses, and Analyses ................................................................................. 87 Comparison of Demographics ....................................................... 94 Literacy Skills Teachers Believe They Are Responsible for Teaching ................................................................................. 99 Emerging Literacy Skills Kindergarten Teachers Believe Should Be Primarily Developed Prior to Kindergarten Entrance ............. 102 Emerging Literacy Skills Teachers Believe Should Be Primarily Developed After Kindergarten Entrance .................................... 104 Emerging Literacy Skills Teacher Believe Should Be Primarily Developed During Early First Grade ........................................... 106 Chi-Square Summary of Perceptions of the Acquisition of Emerging Literacy_Skills as a Function of Teaching Background ............................................................... 107 Chi-Square Summary of Perceptions of the Acquisition of Emerging Literacy_Skills as a Function of Education Background .............................................................. 1 10 ChiSquare Summary of Perceptions of the Acquisition of Emerging Literacy,Skills as a Function of Teacher Participation in Professional Activities ......................... 113 Chi—Square Summary of Perceptions of the Acquisition of Emerging Literacy_Skills as a Function of Classroom Characteristics ........................................................ 1 16 Influential Sources on Kindergarten Teacher Expectations of Children’s Literacy Readiness ................................................ 128 14. 15. 16. 17. 18. Chi-Square Summary of The Most Influential Sources of Kindergarten Teachers’ Expectations of Children’s Literacy Readiness for Kindergarten ......................................... 131 Congruency of Kindergarten Teacher Beliefs of Children’s Literacy Readiness with Others .................................................. 132 Chi-Square Summary of Kindergarten Teachers’ Expectations and Their Perceived Expectations of Others .................................................................................... 134 Most Important Emerging Literacy Skills Children Should Have Upon Kindergarten Entry ................................................... 135 Teacher Agreement With the Michigan Curriculum Framework: Kindergarten Expectations ................................... 138 xi LIST OF FIGURES The Relationship Between Microsystems Within the Greater Set of Systems: A Conceptual Model ......................... 10 Influences on Teacher Expectations of Emerging Literacy Skills .......................................................................... 11 Outside-in and Inside-out Systems .......................................... 24 Phases of Emergent Literacy .................................................. 28 xii CHAPTER ONE INTRODUCTION The word literacy, perhaps more than any other word in education, currently commands much attention and much emotion. While interest in early childhood eduwtion as a whole has markedly increased in the last several decades, literacy development has been most vividly in the spotlight. Whereas early reading and writing skills are now established characteristies of early literacy, contemporary literacy education should also include the attainment of listening skills, speaking skills, and viewing skills (Kostelnik, Sodennan, 8 Whiren, 2004). As the study and articulation of early literacy matures, it is of growing importance that early childhood educators understand early literacy acquisition and the importance it plays in future academic success. The International Reading Association (IRA) and the National Association for the Education of Young Children (NAEYC), in their joint position statement regarding early literacy development, state that in order for every child to achieve high standards of literacy, the responsibility must be shared by families, early childhood programs, schools, and communities (Neuman, Copple, & Bredekamp, 2000). Many studies have determined that there is a direct link between the skills with which children enter school and their later academic performance (Snow, Burns, & Griffin, 1998; Whitehurst & Lonigan, 2001). It is improbable that children who struggle with reading difficulties early will catch up to their peers. Those children whose academic careers begin with inadequate reading-related skills are also more likely to be referred for special education services. Most notably, it is children from low-income families who appear to be at considerable risk for reading difficulties (Snow, Burns, & Griffin, 1998). Early childhood is therefore a crucial time to ensure that children receive developmentally appropriate early literacy experiences. Given that these important emerging literacy skills need to be taught, the quality of emergent literacy depends on the environmental quality and experiences children encounter at home (Hart & Risley, 1995; Purcell-Gates, 1995;Taylor, 1998; Taylor 8 Dorsey-Gaines, 1988), preschool (Neuman, Copple, & Bredekamp, 2000; Snow, Burns, & Griffin, 1998) and school (Morrow, 1993; Whitehurst 8. Lonigan, 2001). Consequently, parents, earegivers, and teachers are responsible for actively supporting children’s emergent literacy development by providing opportunities for children to acquire important emerging literacy knowledge and skills (Bredekamp & Copple, 1997). It is with the support of parents, earegivers, and teachers, as well as exposure to literacy-rich environments, that children effectively advance from emerging to conventional reading. It is important to view children’s literacy development within the context of the family, educational setting, community, and the larger society. Each of these environments is connected to the others and all influence the developing child (Bronfenbrenner, 1998). In order to work collectively for successful literacy development of children, it is beneficial to understand the literacy beliefs of the adults in charge of children’s emerging literacy. Need for the Study Perhaps at no other time in history have Americans better understood the importance of early childhood literacy. Brain development research has supported earlier social research in characterizing the links between early learning experiences and subsequent educational success for children. This growing mass of research has produced a foundation of knowledge upon which curricular and best practices decisions can be made (Roskos, Christie, 8 Richgels, 2003). Organizations such as the National Association for the Education of Young Children (NAEYC), the lntemational Reading Association (IRA) (see Neuman, Copple, 8 Bredekamp, 2000), and the National Research Council (1998) have produced research-based guidelines for education professionals upon which they can base their decisions and practice. However, it may also be observed that perhaps at no other time in recent history have Americans demanded children be taught certain literacy skills at younger and younger ages. A kindergarten teacher of 30 years notes that over the course of about the last six years, kindergarten teachers have felt the pressure of escalating academic expectations across the country. This same concern is heard from colleagues and friends, “Kindergarteners are now expected to learn what had once been in the domain of a first grade curriculum” (DeVault, 2003, p. 90). The story is related to one child in Virginia who would not be promoted to the first grade until she achieved the required reading level. By emphasizing reading at such an early age, teachers must pack in more and more teaching into the already limited day. Yet, while expectations and standards have been heightened, children are coming to school with greater and more diverse needs (Dickinson, 1999). With the increase of public pressure to teach children literacy skills at an earlier age, elementary schools have, accordingly, and as many kindergarten teachers have noted, pushed down first grade curricular expectations. These expectations filter down to the youngest Ieamers. The response to these pressures is constant testing (Dickinson, 1999; Jehlen, 2004; United States Department of Education, 2002). Consequently, particular education theories are once again in a flux. Jehlen (2004) observes that the reaction of some schools to the pressure of raising test scores is to turn away from developmentally based programs in favor of rote, scripted reading programs. Children spend more time sitting and listening to a teacher rather than learning through hands-on, exploration, or experimentation experiences (Dickinson, 1999). As can be seen, the understanding of literacy acquisition and development has grown, societal pressure to increase early literacy has grown, and certain theoretically based programs may now be discarded. Kindergarten teachers are asked to balance this knowledge and pressure, and turn out children who are able to read. It is, then, reasonable to say that more demands are being placed on kindergarten teachers to be successful in teaching early literacy skills. Therefore, an important question must be addressed: Who do teachers believe is responsible for which components of children’s emerging literacy? This study examined those emerging literacy skills kindergarten teachers believed children should acquire prior to kindergarten entrance, those emerging literacy skills that should be acquired during kindergarten, and those emerging literacy skills that should be acquired at the beginning of first grade. Pu_rp_gs_e of the Study The pum of this studeas to i_nvest_rg' ate the perceptions of kindergarten teachers Egardim when your_ig children best aguire emerging literagy skills. Statement of the Prom Explicit abilities necessary for reading and writing develop from direct encounters with both oral and written language over a period of time prior to elementary education. Neuman, Copple, 8 Bredekamp (2000) suggest viewing the acquisition of emergent literacy as a developmental continuum rather than as an all-or-nothing proposition. The continuum extends from birth through the primary grades. By looking at literacy development on a continuum, it is logical that children should learn emerging literacy skills in a smooth and continuous manner. However, there is great diversity in individual children themselves, in children’s oral and written language experiences at home, in childcare settings, and in classrooms; as a result, not all children are at the same spot on the continuum at the same age. Yet, “reading and writing are lifted out of context in schools and become the focus of specific, culturally remote pedagogical attention. Literacy becomes an end in itself, reduced to a hierarchy of interrelated skills” (Taylor, 1998, p.90). If literacy skills are acquired over time, in a somewhat sequential manner, it could be speculated that there would be agreement as to which emerging literacy skills are best acquired before children enter kindergarten, which skills are best acquired in kindergarten, and which skills are best acquired as children transition to first grade. This study attempted to answer the following questions: Which emerging literacy skills do kindergarten teachers believe should be primarily developed prior to kindergarten enhance? Which skills do they believe they are primarily responsible for teaching to young children? Which skills do they believe should be primarily developed in early first grade? Conceptual Framework This research was set within the framework of ecological systems theory (Bronfenbrenner, 1998). A brief explanation of the theory is given followed by a general description of why the theory supports the purpose of this study. Ecoflicel Sfitems Theog Urie Bronfenbrenner (1998) blended together ideas from ecological theory and field theory to create ecological systems theory. Bronfenbrenner sees individual human behavior as a consequence of the interaction between the environment and the person. He contends that the child always develops in the context of imde relationships and that development is the outcome of the interaction of the child’s inherent characteristics with the immediate family and eventually with other elements in the environment. Children develop within a scheme of relationships affecting and affected by four levels of systems: microsystem, mesosystem, exosystem, and macrosystem (Bronfenbrenner, 1998). The micmsystem refers to patterns of activities, roles, and interactions of a child in a particular environment with other particular individuals. For example, a child’s role in the home is that of son/daughter, sister/brother, etc. The mesosystem includes associations beMen two or more microsystems containing the child, such as the relationship between home and school or the relationship between home and childcare. The exosystem describes two or more social settings, at least one of which does not contain the child, but nonetheless influences the child’s immediate setting. Parents’ workplace and health services are examples of the types of formal organizations found in exosystems. The macrosystem includes the laws, customs, cultural values, and resources of the child’s surrounding environment. The precedence that the macrosystem gives to children’s needs affects the support they receive at the inner levels of the environment. For example, if a culture places high expectations and standards for childcare and workplace benefits for working parents, children will be more likely to have favorable experiences in their environments. The environment, according to Bronfenbrenner (1998), is ever changing. Key life events such as beginning school modify existing relationships between children and their environments, thus producing new circumstances. However, changes in the environment are not only imposed on a child, they may also occur within the child. As children get older, they select, modify, and create many of their own settings and experiences. This depends on a child’s physical, intellectual, and personality characteristics, as well as the environmental opportunities available to the child. In ecological systems theory, children are both products and producers of their environments in a scheme of mutually supporting outcomes. Children’s development influences and is influenced by their own characteristics and actions, as well as by the characteristics, actions, and expectations of those closest to them. (Bronfenbrenner, 1998). For example, children’s literacy development is influenced by the literacy characteristics and expectations of those persons present and involved in the child’s immediate environment such as parents, childcare provider, preschool teacher, or kindergarten teacher. These microsystems are in turn influenced by community, societal, and cultural factors. A conceptual model of these system interactions is depicted in Figure 1. NEON-090194 mod_e1 The process-person-context model allows for the study of differences in processes and outcomes as a joint function of the characteristics of the environment and of the person (Bronfenbrenner, 1998). In using this model as a basis for design, this study investigated teacher beliefs as the result of their personal characteristics such as age, ethnicity, number of years teaching kindergarten, and whether they have an early childhood endorsement, among others. Contextual characteristics that may influence kindergarten teachers” perceptions include schools in which they teach, how many children receive free or reduced lunch, and whether the teacher is teaching in a ‘A day session or an all day session. Outside sources in the mesosystem, exosystem, and macrosystem such as assessments used by the school district, in-service training, media sources, professional conferences, and federal legislation may also influence teacher expectations. For the purpose of this study, the focus was on the expectations of kindergarten teachers as a result of the characteristics of the teachers and of the environment. The model in Figure 2 illustrates the dynamic interaction between the teacher and the context. It is this model that helped in identifying differences in teacher expectations associated with different ecological niches. Trme ““‘d Norms/Public 0“ Society community Private ' u--. Childcare 8 Preschool Public Public Head Start 8 MSRP Figure 1. The Relationshi Between +> Monetary Resources Microsystems Within the reater Set . of Systems: A Conceptual Model r:{> Informatron => Values ::::::::::::::;> Weak Relationship ::::-J‘> Weak and Conflicted "' Relationship Past experience Figure 2. Influences on Teacher Expectations of Emerging Literacy Skills 11 Conceptual and Op_e_rational Definitions The following are key concepts encountered in this study: Early Childhood Education Conceptual definition: Includes group programs providing children from birth to eight years with social, emotional, intellectual, language, and physical development and learning (Bredekamp 8 Copple, 1997). Programs in early childhood education include those benefiting infants and toddlers, preschoolers, kindergarteners, and also includes primary programs (Kostelnik, Soderman, 8 Whiren, 2004). Programs such as these offer full day or half-day options, they may be public or private, or specifically targeted at low—, middle, or high-income families, the focus may be enrichment or remediation. Early childhood education programs are conducted by many different types of governmental and community organizations. Operational definition: For the purpose of this study early childhood education regarded the emerging literacy period of ages three-years to six-years old. Emergent Literacy Conceptual definition: Describes the complete process of fluent literacy development (Soderman, Gregory, 8 McCarty, 2005). Children’s literacy acquisition is seen on a developmental continuum beginning at birth, and through meaningful oral and written experiences, changes and develops over time (Clay, 1966). Operational definition: For the purposes of this study, only one phase of emergent literacy was considered and that will was the eariiest phase (i.e. that of emerging literacy). Emerging Literacy Conceptual definition: Describes the eartiest phase or stage of the literacy development process (Soderman, Gregory, 8 McCarty, 2005). It is during this phase that the emergence of literacy skills can be seen. Operational definition: For the purposes of this study, emerging literacy was all of the skills listed on the Emerging Literacy Skills Survey (see Appendix B). Phonological Awareness Conceptual definition: Refers to the hearing and understanding of sounds and patterns in spoken language (Snow, Burns, 8 Griffin, 1998). Included in phonological awareness are such concepts as blending, segmentation, deletion, elision, and rhyming (Adams, 1990). Operational definition: For the purposes of this study phonological awareness consisted of skill numbers 21 and 43, listed on the Emerging Literacy Skills 12 Survey (See Appendix B). Phonemic Awareness Conceptual definition: Refers to the ability to hear and manipulate distinct individual sounds in spoken language (Peterson, Taylor, 8 Hansen, n. d.). Operational definition: For the purposes of this study phonemic awareness consisted of skill numbers 23, 28, and 49 listed on the Emerging Literacy Skill Survey (See Appendix B). Alphabetic Principle Conceptual definition: Refers to the notion that the letters that make up our printed language represent the individual sounds of our spoken language (Adams 1990). Operational definition: For the purposes of this study alphabetic principle consisted of skill numbers 25, 26, and 31, listed on the Emerging Literacy Skills Survey (See Appendix B). Concepts of Print Conceptual definition: Refers to the knowledge that print canies meaning and that the sounds of spoken language can be represented in written form (Peterson, Taylor, 8 Hansen, n.d.). Operational definition: For the purposes of this study, concepts of print consisted of skill numbers 1, 3, 4, 5, 9, 11, 14, 15, 17, 18, 19, 20, 29, 30, 33, 36, 37, 38, 41, 44, 45, 46, 48, and 50, on the Emerging Literacy Skills Survey (See Appendix 8). Oral Language Conceptual definition: Refers to the acquisition of a receptive vocabulary (the number of words a child understands) and the acquisition of an expressive vocabulary (the words a child uses to express himself or herself) (Kostelnik, Soderman, 8 Whiren, 2004). Operational definition: For the purposes of this study, oral language consisted of skill numbers 2, 6, 7, 8, 10, 12, 13, 16, 24, 32, 34, 35, 40, 42, and 47 on the Survey (See Appendix B). Tacher Perceptions of Emerging Literacy Conceptual definition: Refers to the idea that teachers’ classroom decisions stem from their beliefs about Ieaming (Harste 8 Burke, 1980). Operational definition: In terms of this study, teacher perceptions of emerging literacy were their responses on the survey. 13 Research Questions . Which specific emerging literacy skills do kindergarten teachers believe they are primarily responsible for teaching? . Is there any agreement among kindergarten teachers as to which emerging literacy skills they believe should be primarily developed prior to kindergarten entrance? . Is there any agreement among kindergarten teachers as to which emerging literacy skills they believe should be primarily developed after kindergarten entrance? . Is there any agreement among kindergarten teachers as to which emerging literacy skills they believe should be primarily developed at the beginning of first grade? . Do perceptions of the acquisition of emerging literacy skills vary as a function of the demographics of kindergarten teachers? . What sources influence kindergarten teachers’ expectations of children’s literacy readiness? . Are kindergarten teachers’ beliefs congruent with others regarding children’s literacy capabilities? . Which emerging literacy skills do kindergarten teachers believe are the five most essential skills children should acquire by kindergarten entry? . Are kindergarten teacher expectations of skills to be developed during the kindergarten year congruent with the State of Michigan Kindergarten Framework of Skills? Mm . Kindergarten teachers functioned in ways that were compatible with their beliefs. . Teachers’ attitudes and beliefs were formulated by their training and experiences. 14 Summag This chapter included an introduction, need for the study, and purpose for the study. Also included in this chapter was the conceptual framework for this study based on the ecological systems theory. Lastly, operational and conceptual definitions, research questions, and assumpfions were described. A review of the literature pertinent to this study is presented in Chapter II. 15 CHAPTER TWO REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE The review of the literature is divided into five main parts. The first part is an overview of emergent literacy. The second section considers die role of parents as related to literacy development. The third part explores sociocultural effects on the development of emerging literacy. This section includes emerging literacy research based on the characteristics of socioeconomic standing and ethnicity. The fourth part looks at readiness education prior to kindergarten entry. Finally, the review examines kindergarten education. Children’s Emeggpnt Literapy: Traditional Versus Contemmram VIews In order to have a more comprehensive understanding of literacy development, it is helpful to look at what literacy research tells us about how young children become literate. Current views of literacy development are dynamic and noteworthy. Much of this research may shape teacher expectations and may also be considered the foundation for current best practices for many teachers. Contemporary study of children’s literacy acquisition is built on the work of John Dewey (1899; 1902), who saw children’s Ieaming at school in contrast to children’s Ieaming at home. More recently, research by Marie Clay (1966) has supplied the springboard for studying and considering early literacy. Through her work with young children, Clay proposed that children demonstrate emergent literacy behaviors when they read or write in the non-conventional sense. She also emphasized the significant relationship between reading and writing in early literacy development. Up until that time, it was thought that children’s reading 16 must occur before they learned to write. Teale and Sulzby (1986) view the acquisition of literacy as a life-long, continuous process that begins early in life; a complex action with linguistic, social, and psychological characteristics; and as a part of a child’s natural surroundings, acquired both at home and at school. This emergent literacy approach is in contrast to the more traditional readiness approach that views literacy as an all-or-nothing occurrence that starts when children begin school (Lonigan 8 Whitehurst, 1998). The readiness perspective dichotomizes children’s prereading behavior and what is thought to be real reading that children learn in educational settings; whereas, the emergent literacy point of view sees literacy-related actions occurring before and during the preschool years as critical features of literacy development Another differentiation between more traditional approaches to literacy development and an emergent literacy viewpoint is the theory that from birth, there is a parallel and mutually dependent development of reading, writing, and oral language. Children’s exposure to literacy characteristics and events within their immediate environments provides developmental support in the absence of formal instruction. The focus of more traditional approaches is on formal instruction of a number of discrete, separate literacy skills that children must achieve in order to learn to read and write (Morrow, 1993; Nielsen 8 Monson, 1996). The traditional approaches often treat writing as secondary to reading. Emergent literacy is a phrase that describes the process of becoming literate (T eale 8 Sulzby, 1989). This perspective acknowledges that children play an active role in the development of their own understanding of the world through 17 exploration. From both, children progressively become literate through observation and interaction in their environments. Important elements of emergent literacy include the development of reading, writing, speaking, listening, and viewing (Kostelnik, Soderman, 8 Whiren, 2004). Children progress through three general stages of literacy development: emerging, earty, and fluent (Soderman, Gregory, 8 McCarty, 2005). Emerging literacy includes ages approximately birth to five; early literacy includes ages approximately six to eight; and the fluency phase includes ages approximately eight and up. Children will vary in moving in and out of these phases, depending on experience, gender, and other developmental factors. A range of perspectives characterizes the examination of emergent literacy. For example, many studies investigating young children’s literacy related behaviors have revealed that very young children know a lot about print before they enter formal schooling (Clay, 1966, 1991; Durkin, 1966; Harste, Woodward, 8 Burke, 1984; Heath, 1983; Neuman 8 Roskos, 1997; Purcell-Gates, 1996; Read, 1971; Teale 8 Sulzby, 1986; Yaden, Rowe, 8 MacGillivray, 2000). The ground-breaking work of both Clay (1966) and Read (1971) inspired many researchers interested in the field of early childhood education to study print awareness, as well as the reading and writing of young children. Emergent literacy study has also focused on the effect of literacy-rich environments on children’s literacy acquisition (Adams, 1990; Baker, Sonnenschein, Serpell, Scher, et al, 1996; Clay, 1976; Dickinson, 8 DiGisi, 1998; Evans, Shaw, 8 Bell, 2000; Heath, 1982, 1983; Morrow, 1989; Sonnenschein, 18 Brody, 8 Munstennan, 1996; Strickland 8 Morrow, 1989; Taylor, 1998; Taylor, Blum, 8 Logsdon, 1986; Taylor 8 Dorsey-Gaines, 1988; Teale 8 Sulzby, 1986). Literacy-rich environments both at home and at school are key in supporting literacy and preventing reading difficulties. When discussed in the literature, the terms print-rich environment, rich literacy environment, literacy enriched settings, and print environment usually refer to both the presence of literacy materials and children’s engagement In literacy-rich homes, parents and caregivers provide children with opportunities for daily reading, extended discourse (talking or writing), language play, experimentation with literacy materials, book talk, and dramatic play (Burns, Griffin, 8 Snow, 1999; Neuman, Copple, 8 Bredekamp, 1998). Children who are read to learn to listen to and enjoy stories, as well as coming to learn concepts of literacy (Snow, Burns, 8 Griffin, 1998). Literacy-rich classrooms include those in which teachers incorporate the characteristics of literacy-rich home environments, as well as developmentally appropriate practices and experiences, literacy routines, and classroom designs that encourage reading and writing (McGee 8 Richgels, 1996). Literacy-rich classrooms also offer a diverse genre of books, including informational texts (Duke 8 Bennett-Armistead, 2003). Adams (1990), in a review of research on prereaders and how they develop into readers, summarized that children ready to read come to school with large amounts of exposure to print which has parenthetically (or by parent support) taught them about the form and function of language. Most especially, these 19 children have learned phonemic and alphabetic skills, which are the best predictors of later reading success. Yet, Adams also says that children, whose early literacy environments were not as supportive, may still learn to read in classrooms that emphasize direct instruction and explicit teaching of the alphabetic code in a print-rich setting. Taylor, Blum, and Logsdon (1986) agree that children learn best in a language-rich and print-rich environment. They also agree that children from homes that do not offer exposure to literacy events and activities can still develop critical emergent literacy skills it a literacy enriched environment is provided in the classroom. Interestingly, Dowhower and Beagle (1998), in a review of literature concerning the presence or absence of print in facilitating literacy development, found the research base weak. They note that while education professionals stress the importance of a print-rich classroom environment, there is conversely little research on what specific characteristics make up a print-rich environment in terms of literacy materials and how common these materials are in preschool and elementary classrooms across the country. However, Morrow’s (see Dowhower 8 Beagle, 1998) research is noted as unique in its study of the advantages of creating special places for engaging in literacy activities. With the creation of an inviting atmosphere with well-designed literacy centers, Morrow found a significant increase in an interest in books, an increase in the number of children who voluntarily engaged in literacy activities, and an increase in children’s understanding and creations of original stories. Another course of investigation into emergent literacy includes those studies that focus on the acquisition of the components of literacy. For example, there is growing validation of a developmental order to children’s print awareness (see Christie, Enz, 8 Vukelich, 1997, p.58): 1. General concepts about the pum and functions of print. Children recognize, and model in their pretend play, home uses of print, often beginning in the preschool years (Hiebert, 1981). Many studies report incidences of preschoolers making a shopping list, taking food orders, and writing notes and letters. Through similar pretend play and book-reading activities, children, often by the age of thme, also learn to distinguish print from pictures and discover that print has meaning. 2. Graphic awareness (visually mnizipg environmental print, letters, and m Many 3- and 4—year olds are able to recognize pictures in a favorite storybook, logos, or labels in their environment such as McDonalds or KMart signs, Pepsi, and Cheerios (Goodman, 1986). However, children initially recognize print in full context It is not until the age of five or six that children have the aptitude to recognize isolated words (Lomax 8 McGee, 1987). Individual letter recognition may also develop around the same time as environmental print. Some 3—year-olds may recognize one third of the alphabet (Hiebert, 1981) while others will not learn any letters until after kindergarten entrance (Morgan, 1987). 3. Phonemic awareness-the congpt that words consists of a fluence of Men sounds. In acquiring an alphabetic language like English, children 21 must learn that the component sounds of the language are represented by printed symbols (letters or graphemes). In order to grasp the alphabetic principle, children must develop an awareness that language is made up of individual words, and subsequently, each word is made up of syllables and phonemes within each syllable (Snow, Burns, 8 Griffin, 1998). Children develop these skills through language games, most notably rhyming (Soderman, Gregory, 8 McCarty, 2005). Rhyming skills not only help children with spelling; studies indicate that rhyming ability in preschool is a strong predictor of later reading achievement. 4. Letter-sound relationships. Children as young as three (Harste et al., 1984) have shown skill in letter-sound relationships; however, the development of this skill is not seen in many children until the age of five or six. An indicator of letter-sound knowledge development is the use of invented or temporary spelling in children’s writing. While Neuman, Copple, 8 Bredekamp (2000) say that invented spelling will often appear prior to the beginning of kindergarten, Sulzby 8 Teale (1991) note that invented spelling most often does not start until late kindergarten for some children and not until the end of first grade for other children. Whitehurst and Lonigan (1998) have suggested that there are two interdependent sets of skills and processes that make up emergent and conventional literacy. These information systems are called outside-in and inside-out. As Figure 3 illustrates, the outside-in skills characterize what sources outside the printed word that help children understand the meaning of print. Included are vocabulary, phonological-processing skill, conceptual knowledge, story schemas, and semantic sense within contexts. Knowledge of the rules for translating the specific writing they are trying to read into meaningful sounds make up the inside-out elements. These include components within the print that aid the reader in translating print into meaningful sounds. Inside-out features include phonemic awareness, letter knowledge, and links between letters and sounds, decoding strategies, punctuation and sentence grammar, and memory to organize elements into correct sequences. Other emerging literacy skills, including the conventions of print (e.g. directionality, one-to-one correspondence between spoken and written words), the functions of print (e.g. print tells a story or gives information), an understanding of environmental print (e.g. recognizing signs and logos such as McDonalds or KMart), and emerging reading and emerging writing (e.g. pretending to read and write) have been noted in recent research. However, Whitehurst and Lonigan (1998) suggest that “although these abilities and behaviors are sometimes associated with later reading when considered in isolation, research either has not generally supported a direct causal link between them and later decoding skills or has found that these behaviors appear to be better conceptualized as proxy measure for letter knowledge, phonological sensitivity, and oral language, and reflect more exposure to print and other literacy-related activities, or both" (p.5). 23 rContextual UnitsN (e.g., Narrative) \ J i F \ Semantic Units . _. (e.g., Concepts) Outsrde In L J I F \ Language Units _ (e.g., Words) Reading \ J I F N (e fmfl'fp‘) Inside-out L J I F \ Print Units (e.g., Graphemes) J Figure 3. Outside-in and Inside—Out Systems. Emergent and conventional literacy result from children’s ability to employ information from two interdependent information systems: outside-in and inside-out (Whitehurst 8 Lonigan, 1998). 24 As a result of their study of the aforementioned information systems Whitehurst and Lonigan (2001) established three broad categories that appear to have the strongest link to conventional literacy. These categories include one outside-in element (oral language) and two inside-out elements (phonological processing and print awareness). Oral language, while having its greatest impact on children’s later ability to read for meaning, almost certainly has some bearing on the development of decoding abilities, as well. An extensive body of research has established positive correlations and longitudinal constancy between individual differences in vocabulary and other oral language skills and later differences in reading (see Lonigan8 Whitehurst, 1998). Peterson et al. (n.d.) also agree that young children’s skill in oral language will significantly influence their comprehension of the forms and functions of print, their development of a strong reading vocabulary, and their understanding of written material. Hart and Risley (1995) found overwhelming evidence that suggests that children who learn vocabulary more slowty had predictably slower cognitive growth rates, as well. The Hart and Risley study, along with the National Reading Council (Snow, Burns, 8 Griffin, 1998) emphasize that increasing children’s oral language skills could prevent the greater part of reading problems. One aspect of oral language that must be especially emphasized is that of phonological awareness. An abundance of studies on this subject finds that there is a firm link between phonological awareness and reading achievement (Bradley 8 Bryant, 1983; Stanovich, Cunningham, 8 Cramer, 1984; Stahl 8 Murray, 1994; Winsor 8 Pearson, 1992). Phonological awareness is W as 25 the perception of sounds, syllables, and words in language and the ability to understand them in spoken language (Neuman, Copple, 8 Bredekamp, 2000). This includes the awareness of phonemes, onsets, rimes, and syllables (Stahl 8 McKenna, 2000). While the connection between reading achievement and phonological awareness has been established, there is less clarity as to the specific role phonological awareness plays in a child Ieaming to read (Stahl 8 McKenna, 2000). It should be noted that the majority of these studies have been done on phonological awareness and reading. Yet, phonological awareness is also related to spelling. Invented spelling activities, or writing the sounds heard in a word, are shown to be very influential in developing children’s phonological awareness and support the connection between letters and sounds (Adams, 1990). Phonological processing, which refers to activities that call for the use of, the manipulation of, or the sensitivity to the sounds in words, has also been found to play a crucial part of the normal acquisition of reading (Adams, 1990; Wagner 8 Torgesen, 1987). Three interrelated groups of phonological processing skills have been recognized by prior research: phonological sensitivity, phonological naming, and phonological memory (Wagner 8 Torgesen, 1987). Lonigan 8 Whitehurst (1998) note that weak phonological processing skills are a telltale characteristic of poor readers. Examples of important phonological processing skills in the emerging phase include the ability to detect rhyme and supply rhyming words, the ability to immediately recall verbally presented material, and the ability to accomplish serial-naming tasks. Other research has noted that 26 there is a strong link between these three phonological processes and future decoding ability, and that without sufficient intervention, differences in children's abilities are very consistent from late preschool age onward (see Whitehurst 8 Lonigan, 2001). A considerable share of emergent literacy research has been in the verification (of young children’s print knowledge in the preschool years (see Purcell-Gates, 2000). These foundational skills are also critical to children’s literacy development. For example, alphabet knowledge upon school entry is one of the strongest single predictors of short— and long-tenn literacy success (Adams, 1990; Stevenson 8 Newman, 1986). Children must have an understanding of the alphabetic principle, the understanding that there is a systematic relationship between letters and sounds (Byme, 1998). A beginning reader who is unable to recognize and distinguish the individual letters of the alphabet will have trouble Ieaming the sounds those letters represent (Chall, 1967). Letter knowledge is not only important in decoding text; it also has a significant responsibility in the development of phonological sensitivity. Children’s abilities to hear and manipulate phonemes are also associated with higher levels of letter knowledge (Stahl 8 McKenna, 2000; Wagner et al., 1994). The acquisition of children’s literacy is also dependent upon the quality and occurrence of immediate experiences with oral and written language in the early years of life. Children must not only receive these experiences at home, they must also be offered in early education settings. Examples of emergent literacy skills on the continuum of children’s literacy acquisition are illustrated in Figure 4. 27 -participate in reading and writing activities -use pictures, known and familiar letters to represent written language -understand concepts of print such as left-to-right and top-to-bottom orientation, punctuation -retell simple or familiar stories -identify an increasing number of words by sight -participate in reading and writing activities for their own enjoyment -knows letter names and sounds for nearly all letters Emerging Early Fluent Phase Phase pm“ (3'5) (6-8) (8 and up) Children: Children: Children: -begin to play with -continue to play with -read with greater language and participate language. Can rhyme, fluency in rhyming games segment words, and blend words -use comprehension -enjoy listening to and strategies more want to read storybooks -enjoy books efficiently —identify some letters and -orally read with some -read daily and use make some letter-sound fluency reading to research associations topic -use letter-sound -recognize signs and associations, word parts, -use word words in their and context to identify identification environment new words strategies to decode and figure out words -punctuate simple sentences correctly and proofread their own writing -use common letter patterns and critical features to spell words Figure 4. Phases of Emergent Literacy. Emergent literacy is seen on a continuum of development. Young children build a foundation of skills upon which more complicated and comprehensive skills are added until reading and writing skills reach a fluent phase. Adapted from Neuman, Copple, 8 Bredekamp, 2000. 28 Children move from fire emerging phase, filled with exploration of their environment and foundation building, and through the early phase, which is highlighted with the development of the basic concepts of print and experimentation with reading and writing. Most reach the fluent phase, which is underscored with a greater ability to read and write for meaning and pleasure (Neuman, Copple, 8 Bredekamp, 2000). Parents as Teachers Teachers may expect children to bring certain, already developed emerging literacy skills directly to the classroom from home. It is therefore important to examine research that looks at home literacy environments conducive to literacy development. As can be seen, this research explains the positive effects a rich home literacy environment may have on children’s developing literacy. In contrast, however, this research also shows that children from homes not favorable to literacy development may not bring certain expected emerging literacy skills to school. Children whose literacy skill development differs from teacher expectation may also come from homes where parents have a different view of their role in their children’s literacy development. Emergent literacy research (Clay 1966; Read, 1971; Strickland 8 Morrow, 1989; Teale 8 Sulzby, 1986) has changed many educator perspectives on young children’s literacy development and the role that parents play in their development (Edwards, 1991; Heath, 1983; Purcell-Gates, 1995; Taylor 8 Dorsey-Gaines, 1988). Parents, considered to be children's first teachers, are now thought to have the potential to make crucial contributions to children’s developing literacy (Bus, van lJzendoom, 8 Pellegrini, 1995; Hart 8 Risley, 1999; Scarborough 8 Dobrich, 1994; Teale, 1995). As a result, there is much emphasis focused on the home literacy environment. Research has shown there are significant correlations between preschool children’s language skills, specifically vocabulary development, and the home literacy environment (see Whitehurst 8 Lonigan, 1998). While a link between the home literacy environment and the acquisition of other emergent literacy abilities has also been identified (Purcell-Gates, 1996; Teale, 1986), there has been less study of these components. One of the major focal points of home literacy study is shared book reading. A strong body of research has shown that children’s literacy efforts are best supported by adults’ interactions with children through shared book reading (also referred to as reading aloud and storybook reading). Early research by Durkin (1966) noted that children who learned to read before scth entry were read to by parents, siblings, or other caregivers. It was noted that neither socioeconomic level, ethnicity, race, nor IQ differentiated readers from nonreaders. The distinguishing factors were being read to, access to print, parents valuing education, and early writing. While the strength of the relationship has been recently questioned (Scarborough 8 Dobrich, 1994), most researchers and practitioners still agree that shared book reading is an important aspect of children’s literacy and language development (Bus, van lJzendoom, 8 Pellegrini, 1995; Paratore, Melzi, 8 Krol—Sinclair, 2003). The following summary offers a list of the many positive findings of contemporary study as regards reading aloud to young children (Galda 8 Cullinan, 1991; Teale, 1981). The encouraging link between shared reading and literacy development illustrates that reading aloud to children promotes (1) language development in emergent readers (Burns, Griffin, 8 Snow 1999; Chomsky, 1972; Irwin, 1960; Mackinnon, 1959); (2) vocabulary development (Durkin, 1978; Ninio, 1980; Ninio 8 Bruner, 1978; Senechal 8 Cornell, 1993; Senechal, LeFevre, Hudson, 8 Lawson, 1996); (3) children’s motivation to read (Mason 8 Blanton, 1971); (4) Ieaming to read before school entry (Durkin, 1966; Teale, 1978; (5) eariy school reading success (Moon 8 Wells, 1979; Walker 8 Kuerrbitz, 1979; Durkin, 1978); (6) a familiarity with the conventions of print (Clay, 1979; Taylor, 1983); and (7) a metalinguistic awareness of print (Schickedanz, 1986). Bus, van lJzendoom, and Pellegrini (1995) looked at 29 studies of parent- child joint storybook reading and concluded that there are significant effects on children’s literacy and language development. It has been reported that reading aloud to children aids development in oral language, cognitive skills, concepts of print, and phonemic awareness, all of which are important precursors to formal reading instruction (Allington 8 Cunningham, 1996; Hall 8 Moats, 1999; Holdaway, 1979). Children also become familiar with the reading process by watching how others read, and they develop an understanding of story structure. Shared book reading allows children to build a large vocabulary on a wide range of topics, which will later aid in reading comprehension and the development of 31 reading strategies. By hearing a story many times, children’s understandings are reinforced and extended. ' There does not seem to be a direct relationship between shared reading and growth in phonological skills according to recent studies (see Whitehurst 8 Lonigan, 1998). However, it has been discovered that growth in preschool phonological sensitivity is related to parental involvement in home literacy activities such as lap reading and vocabulary development (Lonigan, Dyer, 8 Anthony, 1996). Recently, Senechal et al. (1998) affirmed that kindergarten and first-grade children’s knowledge of written language (i.e., print concepts, letter knowbdge, invented Spelling, and work identification) is linked with parental efforts to teach their children about print, but is not associated with contact to storybooks. They also discovered that children’s oral language abilities are linked with shared reading but are not associated with parent’s efforts to teach about print. Whitehurst 8 Lonigan (2001) point out data that suggest that exposing children to alphabet books may increase letter knowledge and phonological processing. They also identify a dichotomy of studies, some of which have found an association between experiences with word games in the home and the development of phonological processing and some of which do not. Quality parent-child conversation, or extended discourse, has a direct influence on literacy development and also allows for children to be exposed to fisher, more complex vocabulary (Hart 8 Risley, 1995), upon which later reading and vocabulary scores can be predicted (Share et al., 1983). Effective extended 32 discourse compels the participants to be fairly explicit, meaning focused, and able to extend conversation outside of the present (Dickinson 8 Tabors, 1991; Snow, Barnes, Chandler, Goodman, 8 Hemphill, 1991). These types of extended discourses can be seen during shared book reading when book topics are extended by discussion, during mealtime conversations with others, and during fantasy toy play (Beals 8 DeTemple, 1993). Other characteristica of the home literacy environment such as number of books in the home, library visits, and parent’s literacy background are found to be related to children’s vocabulary skills (Senechal et al., 1996). There have been many types of studies executed in order to look at parent perceptions of children’s developing literacy. Yaden (Yaden, Rowe, 8 MacGillivray, 2000) and his colleagues reviewed recent research on parents’ perceptions noting that most studies were process oriented using interviews (Fitzgerald, Spiegel, 8 Cunningham, 1991), observation (Goldenberg, Reece, 8 Gallimore, 1992), focus groups (Neuman, Hagedom, Celano, 8 Daly, 1995), and questionnaires (Hiebert 8 Adams, 1987; Snow, Barnes, Chandler, Goodman, 8 Hemphill, 1991) as methods for gathering data. Gadsden (1998) noted that only a small number of literacy studies concentrate on parents’ perceptions of children’s literacy knowledge or understanding of their earIy literacy, as compared to analyses on parents” influences on emergent literacy. Examples of parents’ perceptions of literacy use can be gleaned from the Baltimore Early Childhood Project (Baker, Sonnenschein, Serpell, Scher, et al., 1996; Sonnenschein, Brody, 8 Munstennan, 1996). Three themes emerged from the findings: (1) Literacy is a source of entertainment, (2) literacy is a set of skills to be purposely developed, and (3) literacy is an essential characteristic of daily life. It is also noted in this study that parents differed in their beliefs of the most effective way of supporting early literacy. While some parents created pleasurable reading opportunities for their young children, others fliought that focusing on skills training was best Parents in Ronald Taylor’s (1995) study of Icelandic families discovered that entertainment was the most reported and observed purpose of reading for both urban and non-urban families. Teale’s (1986) investigation of children from African American, Mexican American, and White families discovered that literacy served many purposes including entertainment, religious, interpersonal, daily living, and school-related tasks. An ethnographic study by Taylor and Dorsey-Gaines (1988) revealed that the five low-income families whose children were successful in school used literacy to gain information in meeting everyday needs, to deal with public agencies, to schedule daily functions, and to learn about activities and events. Reading was also used for educational and recreational purposes. Purcell-Gates (1996) discovered in her study of twenty low-income, diverse families that they, too, used print for many different reasons in their daily lives. She notes, however, that there was a great disparity in the type and frequency of literacy events across homes. Print was not predictably a part of all of the families' lives. Children from homes where more instances of persons reading and writing were included showed more conventional concepts of writing in school. Neuman et al. (1995) discovered a variety of views on literacy Ieaming within a group of African American adolescent parents. As a result of their research of Hispanic mothers of Spanish speaking kindergarteners, Goldenberg, Reese, and Gallimore (1992) found that some mothers believed Ieaming to read started with Ieaming letters and sounds. In contrast, Yaden et al. (2000) observed that a majority of studies concerning parents’ perceptions of literacy acquisition have described findings by cultural groups without further differentiation with regard to individual ability or other factors. Whitehurst 8 Lonigan (1998) claim that there are few quantitative studies that focus on home literacy environments and emergent literacy skills other than children’s oral language. However, they note that studies by Wells (1985) and Crain-Thoreson and Dale (1992) discovered that the frequency of shared reading was linked to concepts of print measures. They also note that Purcell-Gate’s (1996) research found “that families in which there were more higher-level literacy events occurring in the home (i.e., reading and writing texts at the level of connected discourse) had children with a higher level of knowledge about the uses and functions of the written language register, and more conventional concepts about print” (p.856). Children’s reading and writing at home, as well as shared book reading, were found by Mason (1992) to be linked with children’s capacities to label environmental print. As a result of these studies, the powerful role that parents play in the development of children’s emergent literacy can be seen, not only through their interactions with their children but also in creating amenable home conditions for literacy achievement, and through their beliefs regarding the uses and acquisition of literacy. While there has been much attention given to the important role the home, as well as school settings, play in terms of shared reading, reading instruction, and availability of reading materials, there has been much less concentration on seeking information about whether home or school experiences are more vital to literacy development, how literacy beliefs at home and school vary, or how young children cany information from one setting to another. It is important to sort out children’s literacy learning needs at home and at school, and equally important to understand which of these needs teachers believe they are responsible for supporting. By creating a link between home and school beliefs, young children’s literacy needs may be met in a continuous, and more developmentally, socially, and culturally appropriate manner. Sociocultural Effects on the Deflpmnt of Emergipg Literapy A peek into most kindergarten classrooms in America will show children from homes of many different cultures and differing socio-economic status (SES). Yet, there is an expectation that these children should have many of the same emerging literacy skills when they enter school. Contemporary literacy research notes that children may enter schools with differing emerging literacy skills as a result of the differences in home literacy practices as related to cultural and/or economic influences in the home. Guided by the principals of the Vygotskian perspective, the sociocultural perspective on literacy acquisition recognizes that literacy is culturally and socially entrenched (Xu, 2003). Literacy Ieaming is contextual because children learn literacy through activities and events within their unique environment (Goodman, 1985; Heath, 1983; Luke 8 Kale, 1997; Purcell—Gates, 1995; Teale 8 Sulzby, 1986). Xu (2003) explains that children acquire knowledge about language and literacy, by observing how literate people use language and literacy, through everyday interactions with print, including environmental print, and by means of regular literacy practices, such as shared book reading. Literacy systems may vastly differ from one community to another. For example, Heath (1983) reported how children in Roadville, a European American working—class community, children in Trackton, an African American working- class community, and the children of Townspeople, a middle—class, African American and European American community, differed in literacy practices. The Townspeople trained their children to be conversationalists by asking questions requiring specific information. Children were taught to act like readers and writers using an assortment of literacy materials. The homes in Roadville were full of reading material such as newspapers, magazines, church-related material, and children’s books. Parents read bedtime stories to children and extended discourse by asking them questions. Kindergarten-aged children were encouraged to do worksheets and to listen to stories. Children also learned to passively listen and respond to questions in Sunday school. The Trackton children learned literacy as a process of social negotiation with group members. Literacy activities were the responsibility of the whole group, not just one member. Literacy events were full of conversation and information sharing among community members. In short, Heath found children in each town had 37 acquired the literacy skills necessary for involvement in that particular community. However, the acquired skills were not necessarily harmonious with schools. Mismatches between children’s home and school literacy experiences are often the result of differences in literacy practices in different communities. Contemporary research focusing on African American (Heath, 1983), Asian American (Schmidt, 1998; Xu, 1999), Hawaiian (Au 8 Manson 1981), Hispanic (Delgado-Gaitan 8 Trueba, 1997; Volk, 1997), and Native American (Philips, 1983) children suggests that home literacy traditions are often dissimilar to those of the classroom and often go unsupported within the classroom environment. For example, Michaels (1981) noted that first-grade African American girIs’ narratives were more apt to be focused on subjects imbedded by a theme or event. In contrast, European American girts’ narratives were more topic centered or focused on one subject or event. Since the teacher did not understand or value the nanative style of the African American girls, nor did the girls understand the teacher’s discussion approach, the end result was a mismatch of communication styles and fewer chances for the student to practice literate-style discourse. Au (1981; 1993) noticed differences in discourse style between the home and school environments of Hawaiian children. The Hawaiian children jointly executed personal narratives filled with humor and joking, which was quite the reverse of traditional classroom discourse that highlighted individual discourse. The Paratore, Melzi, and Krol-Sinclair (1999) study observed that when parents and teachers shared linguistic and social backgrounds, parents visited with teachers more frequently, asked questions, and sought clarification. Parents whose backgrounds differed from teachers were found to attend formal school meetings and check homework, but rarely initiated contact with the school. It was also noted that when Spanish-speaking parents attended meetings with English-speaking parents, personal contact was again reduced. An ethnographic study by Valdes (1996) of ten immigrant families from Mexico discovered that parents and teachers made assumptions about one another that were factors in the home-school barrier. For instance, the Mexican mothers felt that they were responsible for the moral rearing of their children while it was the teacher’s job to impart school-type knowledge to the children. Teachers assumed that the mothers had only modest interest in the children’s education because the children went to school lacking certain skills such as knowing the alphabet, colors, or numbers. Another point of difference was when the American teachers expected the Mexican mothers to help the children with homework. The mothers would help their children, however they were more likely to focus on aspects of the homework that were different than the teachers. Literacy is a complex, yet essential set of skills for all children living in the United States to acquire in order to achieve success. However, the process is even more difficult for children whose oral and written language at home differs from that of the school. While Spanish speakers are the largest single language minority in the United States (U. S. Census Bureau, 2001), data propose that 39 children whose first language is Spanish are at risk for less than proficient literacy achievement in U. S. schools (Goldenberg 8 Gallimore, 1995; Snow, Burns, 8 Griffin, 1998). For instance, Spanish-speaking children are two times more likely than non-Hispanic Whites to read below age level in English, and are more likely to drop out of high school than Blacks and non-Hispanic Whites (Federal lnteragency Forum on Child and Family Statistics, 2002). While a broad base of literature concerning children’s home literacy encounters exists, it is intriguing that most research seems to have dichotomized literacy study by either looking at White upper- to middle-socioeconomic status (SES) families or low SES families (Gadsden, 1998; Sulzby 8 Edwards, 1993). Children from lower economic backgrounds are apt to be viewed as a homogeneous group (Hammer, Miccio, 8 Wagstaff, 2003). It is noted, however, that besides cultural differences, economic differences exist in the functions of literacy, (Heath, 1983; Purcell-Gates 1995; Teale, 1986), the quality of literacy interactions (Heath, 1983; Philips, 1983), and the incidence of literacy events (Ortiz, 1986). In 1966, Durkin’s work in Chicago with African American families proved to be a springboard for subsequent studies that focused on the ways low- SES parents interact and work with their children to support literacy and they contribute to their children’s literacy development. In her work with eight families from diverse backgrounds, McCarthey (1997) discovered differences between middle-class and working-class families in the quantity and kinds of literacy materials, as well as the purposes of literacy. Despite having fewer resources to obtain information about their children’s 40 classrooms, working-class parents’ knowledge about their children’s classrooms was no different than that of middle—class parents’ knowledge. Parent of both classes placed much importance on literacy activities. Lareau (1989) found that teachers and parents from differing economic groups might have dissimilar beliefs regarding their roles in educating children. This study suggested that while the working-class families frequently stayed away from schools, middle- and upper-class parents made a strong effort to be involved and to communicate with their children’s teachers. Lareau also notes that the middle-class parents were more inclined to believe education is a shared responsibility; whereas, working-class parents believed that education was the teacher’s responsibility. Data from the EarIy Childhood Project (Baker, et al., 1996), which studied a mix of low-income African American and European Nnerican families, and a mix of middle-income African American and European American families, showed that low-income families engaged in fewer print-related activities. Of the activities that these families did participate in, most were intended to develop certain literacy skills. Activities reported by low-income parents included such structured activities as alphabet recitation and flashcards. Yet, some of the low- income families also stressed the entertainment value of literacy and the encouragement of intrinsic motivation. In contrast, middle-income parents preferred to have literacy materials readily available in order to give their children opportunities for forming their own insights into literacy. Entertainment-type literacy activities such as shared reading were also highly encouraged by middle- 41 income parents. This study noted that children who performed better on measures of story understanding and knowledge about print (e.g. letter identification, concepts of print, functions of print) were those children in homes in which there was a greater emphasis on literacy as a source of entertainment rather than a greater emphasis on skills. The Carnegie Foundation, in their 1991 report (as discussed in Whitehurst 8 Lonigan, 1998), revealed that 35 percent of children beginning school in the United States enter with skills and motivation so low as to be at risk for academic struggle. This disclosure signifies a striking “mismatch between what many children bring to their first school experience and what schools expect of them if they are to succeed" (Whitehurst 8 Lonigan, 1998, p.857). In their analysis of social class differences in emergent literacy, Lonigan and Whitehurst (1998) found that a study by White (1982) revealed that the socioeconomic status (SES) of students’ families correlates .68 with academic achievement when schools are ranked by the median SES. The authors also observed that a National Assessment of Educational Progress (1991) study detailed important differences in the reading and writing skills of children as a result of their parents’ income level. Recent literacy studies (see Lonigan 8 Whitehurst, 1998) have also reported that children from low-income families are more apt to (1) be at risk for reading difficulties, (2) be behind in the development of oral language skills, (3) lag in letter knowledge, and (4) be delayed in phonological processing skills before starting school. Social class differences have also been noted in the amount and 42 types of experiences children are exposed to that might support emergent literacy. For example, Ninio (1980) reported that mothers from middle-SE8 groups tended to engage in more teaching behaviors during shared reading than mothers from low-SE8 groups. Hart 8 Risley (1995), in their landmark study regarding the development of vocabulary in young children, found that socioeconomic status of the children’s families accounted for 42 percent of the variance in the children’s rate of vocabulary growth (r= .65), 40 percent of the variance in children’s vocabulary use (5 .63), and 29 percent of the variance in their IQ test scores (r= .54) when they were three years old. Family SES made a tremendous difference in the amount of talking in the family as noted in earty studies (see Hart 8 Risley, 1995). For example, when comparing the average number of utterance per hour that the parents spoke to the child, the authors found the utterances for the high- SES parents was 487 per hour, for the middle-SES parents was 301 per hour, and for the lower SES parents is was 178 utterances to the child per hour. The Hart 8 Risley study proposed that parents “who provide more of the fundamentals, larger amounts of diverse language experience and more encouragement to learn, tend to also add nuances” (p.158), such as listening and encouraging children to expand on their thoughts. These parents are more apt to promote independence by asking for acquiescence rather than demanding it, as well as naming and explaining everything despite the attention or development of the child. 43 Caution must, however, be taken when an attempt is made to portray the homes of poor children as lacking in literacy (Sulzby 8 Edwards, 1993). It has been noted that hidden literacy, or literacy that doesn’t match the expectations of schools, can be found in the daily lives of children and families. For example, Anderson and Stokes (1984), in their cross-cultural study of Iow-SES families found that because the majority of educators are more apt to look for activities such as shared reading and homework assistance rather than the many other incidences of literacy in poor homes, literacy support was misjudged. Taylor and Dorsey-Gaines (1988) also discovered that being a poor young child does not necessarily mean living in an environment totally lacking in literacy opportunities. Despite personal and economic challenges, the families of Black, urban six-year-olds in their study provided literacy activities that in fact supported the successful development of reading and writing skills. It was noted that the literacy experiences in these homes were actually very similar to those Black and White higher SES families studied by Heath (1983). Children are also exposed to literacy in other hidden ways. For example, the Schieffelin and Cochran-Smith (1984) study of Asian refugee families noted that children were successful in reading and writing despite the fact that there was no regularity of exposure to reading or other print materials. However, literacy in the tradition of these families included functional and consequential types of tasks such as personal correspondence through letter writing and filling out forms. Despite parents neither reading to their children nor providing printed material for them, he children still developed the necessary emergent skills for successfully literacy development. Along with the concern that children of differing SES levels may not be offered the same literacy-rich environments and experiences at home, there is also apprehension regarding the divergence in literacy-rich environments between low- and high-SE8 classrooms. In her recent study, Duke (2000) found considerable discrepancies in print experiences and environments when comparing very Iow- and very high-SES first-grade classrooms. The data showed important differences in the amount of print experience, the type of print children experienced, and the nature of the print experience offered to students. For example, Duke found that first-grade classroom libraries in low-SES classrooms were about 40 percent smaller that those in high-SE8 classrooms with fewer books on display, and the high-SES classrooms presented more printed material in the classroom environment. These findings raised high concern regarding the importance of looking at the link between classroom settings and socioeconomic status. If the literacy environments for students in Iow-SES districts are lacking when children first enter school, this might be another negative contribution to the widening achievement gap among low- and high-SES students. The strong link between emerging literacy and later literacy is well recognized. There appears to be strong evidence indicating that children from low—SES homes have relatively lower levels of emerging literacy skills compared to children from middle- and upper-SES families. However, discretion must be 45 used in literacy analyses for the reason that various examinations of home factors as related to literacy development provides evidence that economic levels alone do not determine school success or failure (e.g., Taylor 8 Dorsey-Gaines, 1988) Readiness Education Prior to Kindergarten En_t_ry A look at readiness education prior to kindergarten entry shows an evolution over the past century. Not only have the institutions of readiness education changed, readiness expectations have changed, as well. Progressive study shows that as these programs have expanded, so have the expectations for children. Kindergarten teachers’ expectations for children’s emerging literacy skills may be influenced by the fact that most children now have some group experience before starting kindergarten. Historically, early childhood education programs meant nursery school or center-based care (Kostelnik, Soderman, 8 Whiren, 2004). The term early childhood was used in regard to children five years old and younger. Because first grade was usually a child’s first primary school experience, the distinction between early and later childhood was delineated as such. In recent times, we have realized that significant changes in children’s cognitive, social, and physical development are more likely to take place round seven- or eight-years of age. Early childhood is currently identified as birth to age eight. Current programs serve children with disabilities, as well as children and families from diverse cultural, linguistic, and economic backgrounds (Bredekamp 8 Copple, 1997). A variety of federal, state, and local early childhood programs are offered as half- 46 day or full-day programs. Table 1 illustrates an approximate historical timeframe of readiness thinking. 47 Table 1 Approximate Historical Timeframe for formal schooling, resulting in the creation of the Head Start program. Also seen is the rapid development of center based care for children. reading readiness approach. Kindergartens increasingly engage in formal reading instructions. A ‘pushing down” effect of the first grade curriculum into the kindergarten can be seen. Historical Prevailing Thought on Prevailing Thought on Influential Context Readiness Literacy Education Leaders 1900- 3-, 4-, and 5-year olds Teaching reading to John Watson 1920 (all of whom were young children through Friedrich originally eligible for direct group instruction is Froebel kindergarten) are not seen as inappropriate John Dewey ready for intellectual and perhaps even Maria activity. Primary harmful. Montessori emphasis for early Ieaming was from play and activity, not symbols. 1920- As 3- and 4-year olds Reading readiness is Arnold Gesell 1950 moved out of seen as the product of Sigmund kindergarten and into maturation (i.e., children Freud early childhood classes, must be 6-years old in kindergarten focused on order to learn to read). preparing children for first grade. The concept of maturation prevailed. Curricula focused on readiness activities that supported children’s social and emotional growth rather than stressingggnitive skills 1950- A thrust toward earlier There is a shift away Erik Erikson 1980 education is tied to infant from reading readiness Jean Piaget research that shows that as a product of B. F. Skinner preschoolers know more maturation to readiness Charles than had generally been as the product of Reed thought. Eariy experience. Many Marie Clay intervention is the key to kindergarten and first preparing young children grade teachers take a 48 Table 1 (cont’d). 1980- Readiness is based on Emergent literacy William Teale 1990 development, which in replaces reading Elizabeth turn is influences by readiness. Research Sulzby many environmental shows that young factors, most importantly children demonstrate the home. earIy reading skills before formal schoolipg. 1985- Two viewpoints compete EarIy literacy instruction Bredekamp Present for attention. One view is embedded in age Copple continues to observe appropriate activities. Neuman that readiness is Children's early literacy Lonigan developmental. The Ieaming is embedded in Whitehurst competing view sees a system of oral Morrow readiness as a communication and story maturational process as reading. can be seen in the delay of sending children to Emergent literacy is still kindergarten with the a dominant influence in goal of making sure research, education, and children are ready for policy-making. tasks forrneriy found in first grade. Readiness is seen as preparation for kindergarten. In most states, readiness is determined py age. 2001- The Federal government Snow Present Iegislates the No Child Burns Left Behind Act, within Griffin which literacy programs Soderrnan for preschool-age (Early Whiren Reading First) and Kostelnik kindergarten through Gregory third grade (Reading First) children, are created to enhance reading readiness. These programs influence the types of reading programs and assessments that will be used by local school districts. 49 The current increased demand for early childhood programs can be attributed to an expanding recognition that experiences in the early years have a profound effect on young children’s development (Bredekamp 8 Copple, 1997). Past decades of research confirm that both short— and long-ten'n positive effects on children’s cognitive and social development are gained from high-quality developmentally appropriate early childhood programs (Barnett, 1995, as found in Bredekamp 8 Copple, 1997). A second reason for the jump in demand, and perhaps the most important reason, is the increase in employed families. Approximately 70 percent of mothers with children under the age of six are employed (U. S. Census Bureau, 2003). Children of employed parents attend any number of different settings that vary in goal and quality. The National Center for Education Statistics (2002) notes that in the year 2001, almost 88 percent of children ages 3, 4, and 5 were enrolled in programs, most of which are not in the public sector. While kindergarten attendance is not yet compulsory in all states, about 95 percent of eligible children attended a kindergarten program in the year 2000 (U. S. Department of Commerce, 2001). As a result of the rise in need for child care, and the growing demand for early childhood education programs, most children now enter first grade with some previous experience in group education (Kostelnik, Soderman, 8 Whiren, 2004). With the spotlight on early education more now than ever before, there is an increased need to understand the diverse, as well as the shared roles and goals of early education programs. The history of early childhood programs illustrates the attempt to create a balance between family and society and also a struggle to meet the increasing demands for both education and physical care. Child Care Until only the last several decades, childcare was not considered in the realm of either early childhood education or child development (Caldwell 8 F reyer, 1982). Social reformers and welfare groups first established childcare in large cities for the physical care of infants of working mothers (Read 8 Patterson, 1980). Interest in young children was on the upswing in the nineteenth century due to the increase demand for outside care from working mothers, the medical findings that health was related to sanitary conditions, and the discovery that the death rate for infants whose mothers work was higher than that for infants whose mothers did not work. According to Read 8 Patterson (1980), the first day nursery to open in the United States was the Nursery and Child’s Hospital in New York City in 1854. Nurses cared for children of patients who were now employed. By the turn of the century there were about 175 nurseries for children of immigrant and poor working mothers, most of which were located in settlement houses. Many facilities grew to include toddlers and preschool-age children in their programs. However, with this growth came overcrowding and a drop in good physical care. In order to raise the standards of care, the National Federation of Day Nurseries was created in 1898. The idea of locating programs in public schools was beginning to take hold in the United States early in the twentieth century. While children’s physical health 51 and safety were still the primary goals, attention was being given to the notion that children could also be taught tidiness, good manners, and other social values (Read 8 Patterson, 1980). It was during this time that the child-study movement encouraged nurseries to be opened in private schools, colleges, and universities. The educational programs of these nursery schools also had an influence on childcare. Read 8 Patterson (1980) note that early childcare and nursery schools were alike in structure and program in that both focused on health, nutrition, and social and emotional development using nursery school trained teachers in both settings. However, unlike nurseries, which were meant as a supplement to the upper-middle and upper-class families, childcare was an essential service to immigrants and poor working mothers. While me 1920’s and 1930’s saw a reduction in the number of childcare programs, there was resurgence in the 1940’s at the onset of World War II. Once again, women were needed in the work force for war industries. Federal money was issued through the Lanham Act in 1941 as a support for new group services for families in war impacted areas (Edwards, 2000). The operation of these centers until after the war provided jobs for the considerable number of unemployed teachers, social workers, and nurses. However, most Lanham money was withdrawn by 1946 with no support for children in group settings until the 1960’s. Government sponsored daycare continued from the 1960’s through the 1990’s as a social welfare system designed to support the family life of the poor and as a means to help people move from welfare to work. Edwards (2000) 52 noted that, in contrast, more costly private eariy education programs stressed educational support for children. Preschool While literacy instruction was not traditionally a priority of early preschools, decades of longitudinal research have now established the advantages preschool education can give to children’s literacy Ieaming. At the time, many parents responded to the nineteenth century challenge of early childhood education by sending their young children along with their older children to the common school. This may have been out of concern for the children’s educational development or simply as a relief from caring for the children (Kaestie 8 VInovskis, 1978). The practice of sending young children to the district school with their older siblings revealed the belief that there were no age distinctions and no differentiation of educational needs from older children. As more and more young children attended the common school, two sources of opposition emerged to doom the practice of sending young children to district schools. The first concern was that of finances. Many people felt that it was the parents’ job to educate young children, and including them in the district schools stretched the budget (Strickland, 1982). Another concern was discipline. As the focus and attention of the young children is much shorter than that of the older students, their disruptions were a challenge to district teachers. As a result, many district schools created rules excluding children under the age of five or six, especially in urban and industrialized areas (Strickland, 1982). Consequently, if 53 young children were to be educated outside the home, it would have to be beyond the traditional classrooms of the public school. In contrast to sending children to district schools to be educated, Englishman Robert Owen, an industrialist and social reformer influenced by Pestalozzi’s educational reforms, created a whole new approach to early childhood education. For the concerns of young children, Owen proposed a program to support the unique nature and needs of young children, and thus the infant school was born (Strickland, 1982). It was Owen’s contention that education was a cradle to grave proposition. Young children should not only receive custodial care if needed, but also education that fit the special character of his young. Owen brought his ideas to the United States in the early part of the nineteenth century. However, infant schools faded in the 1930s, partially due to the still popular opinion that young children should be educated at home. By the 19203, a number of teachers who had worked with the Macmillan sisters in England and Maria Montessori in Italy, among others, brought early childhood education ideas and program models to the United States (Read 8 Patterson, 1980). A survey in 1931(see Read 8 Patterson, 1980) noted 203 nursery schools in the United States. Many of these first schools were opened in colleges and universities due to the child-study movement. However, during the economic depression, the Federal Relief Emergency Administration sanctioned the creation of nursery schools to provide unemployed people with work. Unemployed teachers from primary and high schools, social workers, and others were trained at universities and colleges that had laboratory schools. The creation of free nursery schools brought hope that these types of schools would become enduring organizations in the United States. Unfortunately, funding from the WPA for nursery schools stopped in 1943 (Read 8 Patterson, 1980). As the importance of early education became more and more apparent, parents, as well as education advocates, were anxious to secure early foundations of social and educational success for their children. Thus, the early part of the twentieth century also saw the creation of cooperative nursery schools. Besides wanting to ensure an early education program for their children, mothers also sought parent education and child-free time to participate in volunber work. The 19203 saw the spread of cooperative nurseries to various states. Parent participation in the early education of children was an exciting and growing idea. By mid-century the cooperative approach had spread internationally (Read 8 Patterson, 1980). The growth in cooperatives through the 1950s contributed to the overall rise in the number of nursery schools in the United States (Read 8 Patterson, 1980). Despite this rise, there was concern that children in the United States were falling behind children in other countries. There was also increase concern regarding the establishment of preschool programs to the exclusion of poor and mental or physically handicapped children (Almy, 1975). In order to address the rising inequity in early childhood opportunities, the federally funded project Head Start was created in 1965. This program intended to expand opportunities for young children from low income families (Administration for Children and Families, 2002). The purpose was to create a comprehensive program to meet the health, emotional, social, nutritional, and psychological needs of young children aged three- to five-years-old. The program initially served approximately 500,000 children (Read 8 Patterson, 1980). Current Head Start statistics state that almost one million children are currently enrolled. Thirteen percent of those enrolled are children with disabilities, including physical, mental, visual, health, hearing, emotional, speech and language, orthopedic, and Ieaming impairments (Administration for Children and Families, 2003). While the establishment of the Head Start program is an attempt to fill the early education gap for children from poor families, there is still no assurance that all families are able to have access to affordable, high-quality early childhood programs. Neuman et al. (2000) notes that preschool attendance hinges on family income. About 50 percent of American four-year-old children whose family income is less that $20,000 per year attend preschool, as compared to 80 percent of four-year-old children whose family income is more that $50,000 per year. Additionally observed is the fact that the quality of preschool and childcare programs differs to a large extent as a result of insufficient funding. Most programs were found to offer only average quality with about 15 percent of programs rated as good quality (see Neuman, Copple, 8 Bredekamp, 2000). Characteristics of Eam Childhood Pmrams and Teachers The characteristics and quality of programs for young children are of critical importance today due to the fact that more and more children are attending preprimary programs in the United States. Almost 88 percent of 3-, 4-, and 5- year-old children are enrolled in programs in the United States (National Center for Education Statistics, 2002). The status of these programs can be either for- profit or non-profit (Saluja, EarIy, 8 Clifford, 2002). For-profit programs may operate independently or by a national or local chain. Non-profit programs may be affiliated with a public school, a religious organization, or another type of non- profit program such as the YMCA. In their 1997 nationwide survey, Saluja, et al. (2002) found that approximately 8 percent of the programs were Head Start programs, 16 percent were in public schools, 25 percent were independent non- profit or other public agency programs, 22 percent were associated with a religious organization, and 29 percent were for-profit programs. While the majority of young children attend a program in the United States, public funding for these programs is lacking. An lntemational comparison of public spending reveals that the United States provides fewer early childhood education programs despite having more resources than similarly developed countries (see Edwards, 2000). For example, 95 percent of 3-year—olds and all 4-year-olds in Belgium are in public schools or in publicly funded programs; France provides free schooling for children ages three and older, with preschool teachers having the equivalent of a master's degree in Earty Childhood Development, and students of preschool education receiving a stipend and fiee college education if they agree to teach for five years; 87 percent of Italian 3- to 5-year olds are in school; and the government in Denmark subsidizes childcare for children from six months to age seven, with professional teachers and low child-to-teacher ratios in each classroom. 57 It is agreed that high-quality early childhood programs have potential positive effects. Several significant studies of childcare and preschool programs (see Bredekamp 8 Copple, 1997) point out that high-quality experiences are not the standard. Only 15 percent of programs offered the good quality that was necessary for children’s health, social, and cognitive development. In fact, 12 to 20 percent of children were found to be in settings thought to be detrimental to their social and cognitive development, as well as dangerous to their health and safety. Even more disturbing is the finding that 30 to 40 percent of infants and toddlers were placed in unsafe environments (Cost, Quality, 8 Child Outcomes Study Team, 1995). Varied levels of classroom quality are found not only in childcare and preschool, but also in children’s eariy elementary school experience (Bryant, Clifford, 8 Peisner, 1991; Durkin, 1987). In a statewide observational study of kindergartens, Durkin (1987) remarked that there is disparate quality of kindergarten classrooms. For example, he found teachers tended to rely on whole-group phonics instruction as their sole teaching strategy despite assessment results that pointed to a variety of individual differences in children’s literacy skills. Instead of using different teaching strategies such as small-group or individual work to support children’s development, the teachers viewed children who could not learn well with the one method as unready for first grade. In order to improve the quality of early childhood education programs, exceptional training and adequate compensation for teachers is necessary (Katz, 1997). Despite the fact that there is a positive relationship between the education and consistency of staff and the quality of care, only a small percentage of childcare teachers have four-year or advanced degrees, and the staff turnover rate is as high as 25 to 50 percent per year (National Center for Early Development and Learning, 1997). In most states, certification for preschool teachers includes an early childhood teaching certificate or endorsement when the preschool programs are located in the public school. The 1990 Profile of Child Care Settings (Kisker, Hofferth, Phillips, 8 Farquhar, 1991) found that 47 percent of teachers had four-year college degrees, 13 percent had an associate’s degree, 26 percent had some college, 13 percent had graduated from high school, and 1 percent had less that a high school degree. The data also showed that 88 percent of teachers in public schools had a college degree or graduate degree, whereas 50 percent of teachers in religious settings and 45 percent of teachers in Head Start had college degrees. Saluja, et al. (2002) found similar data in their study. They also note that many states are in the process of creating new policies pertaining to teacher education requirements which they hope will lead to a more highly educated early childhood staff. It could, therefore, be suggested that preschool teachers may not be aptly equipped educationally to support children’s needs for early literacy Ieaming. Head Start classroom teacher qualifications for the state of Michigan (under Head Start Act Sec. 648A) include a minimum of an associate degree in EarIy Childhood Education or a field related to Early Childhood Education with experience in teaching preschool children (Administration for Children and Families, n.d.). However, there is alternative credentialing for classrooms that do 59 not have a teacher that meets the minimum degree requirements; one teacher must have a Child Development Associate (CDA) credential that is appropriate to the children being served; or a state-awarded certificate for preschool teachers that meets or exceeds the requirements for a CDA credential; or a related child development degree and a state-awarded certificate to teach in a preschool program. General elementary school certification in the state of Michigan, including earIy childhood teachers, requires a minimum of a bachelor degme, the passing of the basic skills examination, the elementary certification examination, and an exam in early childhood (Legislative Counsel, 2003). However, not all states require teachers to pass certification exams. As of 1997, six states did not require an exam for teacher licensure (National Association of State Directors of Teacher Education and Certification, 1998). Those states that do require teacher testing for certification vary in exam and licensure standards. Each state is responsible for establishing individual standards for licensure test passing cutoff points, their choice of tests or no test at all, a re-take policy, and which states’ teacher licensures they recognize (Gitomer, Latham, 8 Ziomek, 1999). It is a challenge for higher education to offer teacher training models that will prepare students to pass teacher certification tests, teach students the skills necessary to cultivate appropriate development in young children, and, develop collaborative programs with local early childhood programs to enhance quality of care of children and support the development of early childhood teachers as professionals. It should also be noted that historically, the majority of early childhood teachers have been White females. This characteristic seems to still be prevalent in the early childhood setting. For example, a study of teachers of 3- and 4-year-olds by Saluja, Early, 8 Clifford (2002) found that 78 percent of teachers were White, 10 percent were Black or African American, 5.7 percent were Hispanic or Latino, 3.8 percent of mixed race, 1.1 percent Asian or Pacific Islander, and .9 percent American Indian or Native Alaskan. They also found a smaller percentage of White teachers in Head Start programs than other types, and a higher percentage of Hispanic or Latino teachers in the public schools than in other programs. The average age of these same teachers was 39 years, with 99 percent of teachers across programs being female. Today, a largely White, female population, most of whom have a four-year or advanced degree, characterizes kindergarten teaching in the public school system. Teachers in the public schools most likely have to pass a teacher certification exam, for which they may or may not be adequately prepared. Certification of preprimary teachers is not required. However, financial compensation is low in nonpublic early education settings, job turnover rates are greater, and teachers’ academic skills are more dubious than in public school settings (Edwards, 2000). Kindergarten Education: Yesterday and Today By looking at kindergarten education over time, an understanding of the current position of kindergarten teachers may be gained. Historical thought and research gives way to current thinking and societal pressure resulting in the 61 downward push of the elementary curriculum. These pressures may perhaps shape teacher expectations for children’s literacy development. Friedrich Froebel’s late nineteenth-century idea to create a children’s garden, or kindergarten, specifically addressed the educational needs of young children based on their level of development (Downs, 1978; Morrow, 1993). Froebel, a student of Pestalozzi, based his educational system on the notion that mankind was the physical personification of God’s reason. Because Fmbel believed that all things unfolded their nature according to a divine prepattemed plan, in order to best educate a child one need only discover the connection between the child’s outer and inner worlds (Shapiro, 1983). Froebel saw the need to divide die process of early education between birth and age six into separate stages of mental and physical development: infancy, early childhood, and childhood. The development at each stage determines educational responsibilities. It was the early childhood years of 4 to 6 that most interested Froebel. He searched for a program that would serve as a link between the loving, yet permissive home environment, and the submissive atmosphere that the school discipline required (Shapiro, 1983). Unhappy with the current offerings of the time, he created the kindergarten. The idea of the kindergarten was to allow children of like age to learn together outside the influence of the family and school. Children would be able to explore their worlds naturally through a curriculum based on games and occupations. Formal reading instruction was never a part of the curriculum (Froebel, 1889). 62 Believing educated teachers to be an integral part of the kindergarten program, F roebel insisted that the teachers or kindergartners be particularly trained in child development and care. The kindergartner (is. the teacher) was responsible for children’s social, physical, and spiritual development in a community of peers (Palmer, 2001). The fundamental nature of teaching was adjusting the topics to be learned to the child’s present stage of development. The term child-centered originated with Froebel and continues to be an important theme in early childhood education (F roebel, 1889). Froebel’s emphasis on play has also had a great impact on practice. It is play, Froebel argued, that is the child’s primary method of gaining knowledge about the world. Initially, kindergarten served both a philanthropic and religious purpose in the United States (Hill, 1996). Most kindergartens were set up in the most run-down parts of cities where many poor and immigrant children lived. Cultured and eager women, trained in philanthropy, supported normal schools, taught in the morning and spent afternoons as social-welfare agents assisting parents in finding employment, medical services, and even dental services for the poor families they served. As a result of their financially overburdened state, philanthropic boards resorted to pleading with the public schools to integrate the kindergarten as the foundation grade into the public school system. However, the kindergarten classroom did not acclimate smoothly into the public school system. After the financially troubled philanthropic agencies convinced the public school system to take over the burden of kindergarten financing and administration, kindergarten teachers discovered that there was no longer time to play the important role of social-welfare agent. Teachers no longer had time to play the role of medical inspector, social caseworker, or visiting teacher (Hill, 1996). The kindergarten classroom also saw a considerable increase in the number of students per classroom with the introduction of the double session. Gone were the opportunities for parent guidance and assistance in the afternoons. There was also much resistance by first grade teachers to accept the new role kindergarten played as the child’s first school experience. By the mid-19203 approximately 12 percent of American five-year-olds were enrolled in kindergartens. While the adoption of kindergarten into the public school system saw a profound change in the education of young children, so too did the upsurge of professionalization of early childhood education (Shapiro, 1983). A steady rise in important early childhood research and education centers by influential American universities gave way to the emergence of new voices concerning early childhood education. Many prominent institutions of higher education including the Iowa Child Welfare Research Station, the Yale Clinic of Child Development, and the Child Welfare Institute at Teachers college, Columbia University were now centers of child study. About this same time, the child-centered theories of John Dewey and William Kilpatrick were developing in contrast to the behavior theories of John 8. Watson and Edward L. Thorndike at Columbia University. Dewey (1990) and those with similar theories emphasized the importance of social interaction and how each in his own way is a part of the social whole. To have a democracy, children must be taught to interact with others in society. This was in contrast to the behaviorist view of planned goals and instruction. In order to reconcile the two schools of thought, Patty Smith Hill (Shapiro, 1983) created the conduct curriculum, which combined Dewey's social goals with Thomdike’s Ieaming objectives. This integrated curriculum was well accepted by the preservice teacher graduates of the time. The 19303 brought a new, emerging leader in child development theory. Arnold Gesell, a psychologist who began as a student of G. Stanley Hall, believing intelligence to be fixed and development to be predetermined, proposed to ascertain a pattern of normal growth in young children (Shapiro, 1983). Gesell developed his theory through observational studies of growth and behavior of children from good homes or of high-socioeconomic status. The basic tenet of his theory says that as long the infant or child is properly cared for, maturation advances in an orderly fixed rate. Thus, he supported the notion that maturation is the most important factor in Ieaming to read. Children should not learn to read until they are deemed developmentally ready, usually around the age of six. However, Gesell was criticized for not including individual differences among children in his studies, as well as making over-generalized statements about development (Thomas, 2000). While Gesell’s innovative child-study methods still play an important role in the observation of children, his theories generally lost favor late in the twentieth century to more comprehensive theories. The concept of maturational readiness remained popular through the mid- twentieth century. Curricula were based on the developmental norms for the age ranges in question. The emotional well-being of children was of emphasis during this time as a result of Freud’s discernment that childhood education might assist in the prevention of mental illness, as well as Dewey’s continued influence on the concentration of allowing children opportunities to interact socially and shengthen their emotional well-being (Kostelnik, Soderman, 8 Whiren, 2004). The end of WorId War It brought another change in early childhood education. The biggest trigger in reassessment of early education in the mid—twentieth century came with the launch of a Russian sputnik in 1957. This put the American space program embarrassingly well behind the Russian program. Many blamed education as the cause. American education was now under the microscope and early childhood education was the focus. Parents feared that kindergarten was lacking in intellectual stimulation. “The shift toward preparation in the kindergarten for later academic skill, foreshadowed in the ‘reading readiness’ programs of the 19203, was accelerated” (Shapiro, 1983, p. 194). Progressive kindergarten education continued to characterize curriculum development in the light of encouragement of growth and development, as well as the realization of specific Ieaming. Yet, as the progressive education movement faded, less emphasis was put on development and more concern focused on achieving specific Ieaming outcomes (Spodek, 1996). The readiness notion of the kindergarten of the time is well illustrated by Gans, Stendler, and Almy (1952) as the 3R’3 Curriculum: ‘l'he3R’sapproachhasnotonlyprevailedintheprimarygrades, butithasreacheddowninto the five-year-old kindergarten. Counting, some writing and reading readiness activities chiefly in thefonn ofworkbook exercises have been typical experiences in kindergartenwhere this curriculum has been in operation. Under such a setup the kindergarten is seen as a year of settling downtorchildren, ofadjustingtositting stillandfollowing directions, sothattheywil be better prepared fora more rigorous attack on the 3R3 during first grade (p.80-81). A3 a result of this turn in focus, educators began to seek out updated theory and methods that could answer not only the call for preparation in the kindergarten for later academic skills, but that could also provide solutions to the widening gap between the educational opportunities of rich and poor children in America. The work of Swiss psychologist Jean Piaget, while previously recognized, now came under close consideration of psychologists and educators (Spodek, 1996). In the tradition of Dewey, Piaget’s focused on cognitive development, and his understanding that children are capable of only certain types of intellectual tasks at certain stages, helped create an early childhood curriculum that focuses on decision making, problem solving, self-discipline, goal setting, planning one’s own activities, and cooperating with teachers and peers in evaluating leaning (Morrow, 1993). He also stressed that Ieaming takes place when the child interacts with peers and adults in a social setting as they act upon the environment Especially useful in the classroom were the distinctions Piaget made between imitation (the child accommodating to the extemal world) and play (the child assimilating new information into the existing frameworks) (Shapiro, 1983). A3 Piaget developed only a theory of development and not a program of education, early childhood advocates had to create their own programs using Piaget’s hypotheses of early Ieaming and development (Kostelnik, Soderman, 8 Whiren, 2004). As a result, mid-century earIy childhood programs were based on a variety of adaptations. These included strictly following Piagetian theory, but also incorporated theories focusing on the interaction between both 67 environmental and biological influences such as Erikson’s emotional development theory, Bandura’s social Ieaming theory, Dewey’s theory of play, and Vygotsky’s theory of language and culture. This incorporation of theories and practices established what is now recognized as the interactionist approach (Kostelnik, et al., 1999). The spread of the interactionist approach inspired early educators to rethink their role in the classroom, as well as take a new look at the use of materials in the classroom, and the goal of early education (Kostelnik, et al., 2004). However, there was also a growing trend evident in many early childhood classes and particularly in kindergartens that put excessive importance on rote learning and whole group instruction of a specific set of academic skills (Bredekamp 8 Copple, 1997). Considerable thought was also being given to the methods used to test, place, and retain young children. Because young children were being subjected to readiness and screening methods based on the same narrow academic goals that typified the elementary curricula, a growing number of children were distinguished as not ready for kindergarten or first grade and were subsequently referred to transition classes, retained, or denied entry. As a result of this growing multiplicity of programs, and an expanding movement toward more formal, academc instruction of young children, as seen in the downward push of the elementary curriculum, position statements on developmentally appropriate practices were published by the National Association for the Education of Young Children (NAEYC) (Bredekamp, 1987). The key position was that programs intended for young children should be based . on what is known about the development of young children (Bredekamp 8 Copple, 1997). The guidelines for developmentally appropriate practices set by the NAEYC were helme not only for daily program decisions nationally and internationally, many state departments of education in the United States have used the guidelines to augment policy and program evaluation choices, as well (Bredekamp 8 Copple, 1997). A joint position statement from the lntemational Reading Association (IRA) and the National Association for the Education of Young Children (NAEYC) was adopmd in 1998 (Neuman, Copple, 8 Bredekamp, 2000). This important document was intended to guide the establishment of developmentally appropriate settings and practices that best contribute to the development of children’s literacy. The statement was aimed at not only teachers of young children in schools and early childhood programs, but also anyone who works with or can contribute to the development of children from birth through age eight. It is through these position statements that a call was made for educators, families, and caring communities to come together to share the responsibility in educating young children in ways that are developmentally appropriate. Developmentally appropriate practices (Bredekamp 8 Copple, 1997) in literacy education take into account what is known about children’s development and Ieaming, individual children’s progress along the continuum of literacy development, and the social and cultural contexts in which children live (Neuman, Copple, 8 Bredekamp, 2000). 69 Today, kindergarten expectations are incurring changes in the United States based on legislation passed in 2002 called the No Child Left Behind Act of 2001 (NCLB) (U. S. Department of Education, 2002). This legislation is meant to make states, school districts, and schools accountable for the accomplishments of individual students. It calls for states to create immediate standards in math reading, and science by the 2005-2006 school year. Students are to be assessed based on those standards, and in turn, school districts and schools will be assessed on the performance of their students on these standardized tests. While Michigan has already implemented the Michigan Curriculum Framework (Michigan Department of Education, 1996) as a guide for curricular standards, the twelve English Language Arts Content Standards found are broad cuniculum statements that describe what students should know and be able to do by the time they graduate from high school. In order to comply more comprehensively with the NCLB act, the Michigan Department of Education sponsored the development of the Grade Level Content Expectations (GLCE). Aligned with the state content standards and benchmarks, these expectations are more concrete expectations for each grade level and aid teachers in preparing students for grade level assessments. Appendix A shows the Grade Level Content Expectations for Kindergarten English Language Arts (Michigan Department of Education, 2004). Wrthin the NCLB act are the EarIy Reading First (ages 3 to 5) and the Reading First (grades K to 3) initiatives. These programs focus on reading readiness, professional development for teachers, as well as guide school 70 decisions on which reading and assessment programs to purchase. Schools must supply programs that are based on proven scientific research or scientifically based reading research (SBRR). The National Reading Panel was asked by Congress to establish which scientifically proven principles help children learn to read (Berger 8 Gunn, 2003). As a result, the panel suggested that instruction, assessment, and professional development should concentrate on phonemic awareness, phonics, vocabulary, fluency, and comprehension. Most notably, the panel supported explicit instruction in phonics and phonemic awareness. Reading First has thus reenergized phonics instruction, much to the dismay of whole language advocates. Yatvin, Weaver, and Garan (2003), in their review of the Reading First initiative, say that this plan comes from an “incomplete and flawed research base, the National Reading Panel Reports, and from government documents and regulations that substantially misrepresent the Report’s findings” (p. 28). For example, they site that there is no evidence in the National Reading Panel Report, or anywhere else, that children must develop phonemic awareness or phonics before they begin to read print. Nor was it confirmed that there is scientific research to support the use of commercial reading programs, which may even have disadvantageous effects on children’s literacy development. Interestingly, the National Reading Panel does not even recommend any commercial programs. Yet, as a consequence of this deficient research and a rush to implementation, there is widespread use of scripted, cookie-cutter type commercial reading programs in schools. 71 Another concern with NCLB is the mandated assessment component (Goertz 8 Duffy, 2003). There is great concern and controversy about the tests themselves and the use of the results. While most states have begun restructuring efforts that comply with the performance standards and mandated assessments, too frequently the assessment outcome becomes the and rather the means used to improve instructional programs. Gilmore (2003), in her comparison of literacy assessment and IQ scores, reasons that the move to mandate standardized testing relegates literacy to a numbers game that includes reading scores, percentiles, and grade levels, just as IQ scores do. She surrnises that literacy assessment of this type, just like IQ scoring, inhibits access to literacy and creates another barrier to literacy for certain social groups. The NCLB legislation has also raised questions about how to deal with the literacy assessment programs schools already have in place (Berger 8 Gunn, 2003). Changing programs can have a less than positive effect with schools and teachers. For example, Berger 8 Gunn (2003) discuss that a reading researcher at the University of Michigan has found that in Michigan, 20,000 teachers have been trained to use a literacy assessment program called the Michigan Literacy Progress Profile (MLPP). This particular program was created and researched in Michigan. However, the program cannot be used in Reading First programs in Michigan because it is not an (U. S.) Education Department endorsed program. This has caused much resentment among teachers who must now go through another training process with a new Reading First plan, one that they view as an unrelated addition, not linked to the MLPP. It should be noted that group- 72 administered, nomi-referenced standardized testing is inappropriate for young children. It is recommended that this type of conventional assessment be done no sooner than the fourth grade. Recommendations call for more feasible assessment of children’s literacy development should be done using a variety of indicators across a continuum of time (Bredekamp 8 Copple, 1997). The success or failure of the recent NCLB initiative hangs in the balance. Kindergarten curricula may seemingly undergo more changes in the coming years as a result. However, despite changes in education mandates, children's literacy will still be found to develop along a certain continuum. Teachers will still teach literacy skills to children who come to school with a variety of emerging literacy skill levels. Therefore, it is important for kindergarten teachers to be aware of the progress of children’s literacy development. A survey of kindergarten teacher perceptions of when certain literacy skills should be developed will contribute to our understanding of how aware kindergarten teachers are of Iiteracy’s developmental continuum. Summag The development of emergent literacy skills is a continuous process that begins at birth and continues throughout life. In order to ensure the reasonable and appropriate development of these important skills, children must be immersed in a wortd of language and print in their immediate environment. 73 Contemporary research has helped unfold an understanding of literacy development Studies have shown that an environment in children’s homes and early childhood education settings that is rich in print and language is of the utmost importance for the development of early reading and writing skills. Focus has been given to the important role the home, as well as the preprimary environment play in terms of shared reading, reading instruction, and availability of reading materials. Yet, much less consideration has been given to seeking information about whether home or school experiences are more vital to literacy development, how literacy beliefs at home and school vary, and how young children carry information from one setting to another. Many social and cultural factors play important roles in both reading and writing development. Teachers should be aware of how these factors augment the level of opportunity, the richness of the environment, and quality of care offered to young children, all of which in turn have a critical impact on literacy development. Because literacy systems vastly differ from one community to another, teachers of children age’s three to five, as well as teachers of children in kindergarten through third grades, must be especially sensitive to the literacy expectations in their communities. Early childhood education programs, philosophies, and teachers have all seen change over the last century. Numerous studies have helped create a better understanding of how young children develop, which have subsequently aided in the formation and establishment of more appropriate approaches to teaching and Ieaming. Nonetheless, there are still no set standards for early childhood teacher 74 preparation. Given that teacher knowledge and practice varies with both teacher training and personal beliefs, further study is necessary to discover which emerging literacy skills early childhood education teachers believe they are primarily responsible for teaching. The next chapter describes the methodology to achieve the study of kindergarten teacher beliefs. 75 CHAPTER THREE METHODS The purpose of this study was to investigate the perception of kindergarten teachers regarding when children best acquire emerging literacy skills. Research Desk] n The design of this study was survey research. This study was a cross- sectional design taken during the spring of 2004 from the population of kindergarten teachers in Ingham County Variables The dependent variable in this study was kindergarten teacher perceptions about emerging literacy skills. The independent variables included personal demographics, teaching background, teacher education background, professional activities, and classroom characteristics. Subjects The study population was comprised of all 131-kindergarten teachers in the twelve scth districts that make up the Ingham lnterrnediate School District in mid-Michigan. These public school districts included the rural districts of Dansville Schools, Leslie Public Schools, Stockbridge Community Schools, and Webberville Community Schools; and the urban school districts of East Lansing Public Schools, the Lansing Public Schools, Haslett Public Schools, Holt Public 76 Schools, Mason Public Schools, Okemos Public Schools, Waverly Community Schools, and Williamston Community Schools. The designation of rural and urban is taken from the United States Census Bureau 1995 definition, which states that “urban” consists of places of 2,500 or more persons (United States Census Bureau, 1995). The sample population was comprised of 42 kindergarten teachers from nine school districts, two rural districts and seven urban districts. Ninety-eight percent of the sample were female teachers of whom 92.9 percent were White, five percent were African-American, and two percent were Hispanic. This composition is similar to national characterizations of kindergarten classrooms (Edwards, 2000). This put the actual response rate of the surveys at 32 percent. According to Baruch (1999), a response rate of 55.6 with a standard deviation of 19.7 was found to be the average response rate in academic studies. Three school districts, Haslett, Leslie, and Webberville had no kindergarten teachers return the survey. Surveys in these districts may not have been returned due to possible principal interference, poor timing of the survey, or lack of interest in participation by the teacher (Baruch, 1999). The Ingham County Intermediate School District (IISD) area was chosen for this study as it included diverse groups of teachers and students among and within the schools in the IISD. As the IISD showed an early interest in the formation of this project, the Supervisor of Early Childhood Education-School Development Services was contacted by telephone to confirm the IISD’s continued interest. Upon confirmation of the IISD’s willingness to participate in this study, a list of all of the kindergartens in each of the participating schools was created. An initial contact with the superintendent of each district was made by the IISD as a means of obtaining consent to implement the project in the district. Upon district approval, the principal of each elementary building was sent a letter confirming the consent Each principal received a copy of the survey and letter that will was sent to the teachers (See Appendix D for a copy of the principal letter). Teachers who participated were eligible for one of four gift certificates to be given away. The consent letter attached to the returned survey was separated from the survey and put into a container from which the four winners of the gift certificates were drawn. Instrumentation One instrument was used to collect data. The Emerging Literacy Skills Survey was designed specifically for this study. The questionnaire was divided into four sections. The first section collected demographic information such as personal demographics, teaching background, teacher education background, professional activities, and classroom characteristics. The second section asked teachers to indicate the sources that have been most influential in forming their expectations for children’s readiness for kindergarten and whether their expectations of what children are capable of doing are congruent with other people and standards. The third section asked the teachers to list which five 78 emerging literacy skills they thought were the most important for children to have upon entry into kindergarten. The fourth section asked teachers to choose which emerging literacy skills they thought children should primarily develop prior to kindergarten entry, which emerging literacy skills they thought children should primarily develop during the kindergarten year, which emerging literacy skills should be primarily developed during first grade, and whether this belief was consistent with district demand. The variables were selected from the Michigan Department of Education, Michigan State Board of Education, Head Start Child Outcomes Framework, High/Scope Child Observation Record, National Association for the Education of Young Children, Lansing School District Curriculum Benchmarks for Kindergarten, and Kostelnik, Soderman, 8 Whiren, 2004. The emerging literacy skills listed in the fourth section of the survey clustered into six groups: phonological awareness (2 skills including recognize rhyming words and breaking words into syllables); alphabetic principle (2 skills including identify letters in the alphabet and associate letter names to their sounds); phonemic awareness (3 skills including identify beginning/middle/end sounds); oral language (15 skills including retell story details and use descriptive language); concepts of print (24 skills including recognize own name in writing and understand print contains a message); and communication abilities (3 skills including develop interest in literacy experiences and listen attentively to stories). The skills in this section were listed in random order. Each skill was written on an individual piece of paper and drawn out of a container one at a time by a third 79 party. The random listing of the skills was meant to have the teachers consider each skill individually and not influence the teacher by the order in which it was presented. A reliability analysis was run yielding an alpha coefficient of .8916. The full survey can be seen in Appendix B. The tool has construct validity as it was based on emergent literacy theory, particularly the works of Neuman, Copple, 8 Bredekamp (2000), Clay (1966), Kostelnik, Soderman, 8 Whiren (2004), Whitehurst 8 Lonigan (1998), and (T eale and Sulzby (1986). The foundation of this theory notes that children’s reading and writing abilities develop concurrently and interactively through experiences in reading and writing. Emergent literacy theorists believe oral language plays a key role in the development of emerging literacy skills of young children. Many children begin to learn to read and write well before they enter formal schooling by, among other things, vocabulary development, listening to stories being read aloud, noticing signs and labels in their environment, and experimenting with writing tools (Clay, 1966; Teale 8 Sulzby, 1989). Many children exhibit emergent literacy behaviors such as pretending to read a favorite story to a teddy bear or scribbling a letter to Grandma. Each survey item and its source can be seen in Table 2. The present survey was adapted from a previous survey constructed by the researcher. The original survey was used in an investigation of kindergarten teacher perceptions in the Lansing School District in Lansing, Michigan (T helen, 2002). Prior to the current study, the present survey was tested for usability and validity in a pilot study. Five kindergarten teachers from Gratiot County completed the survey. It took an average of approximately fifteen minutes for teachers to finish the survey. When the teachers were done with the survey, there was discussion as to the ease of taking the survey. The teachers noted that the directions were understandable and easy to follow. Some teachers thought that the percentages in the demographics section were difficult to specifically ascertain. As a result, the word “estimate” was added to the directions in this section. In regards to whether there were any emerging literacy skills not listed that the teachers’ thought should be listed, there were none suggested. It should be noted that there was an opportunity for teachers to state what they believe are the five most important emerging literacy skills a child should have upon kindergarten entry. This section allowed for teachers to note any skill(s) that might be important to literacy development, but that were not listed on the survey. There were no clarifications asked for, nor were there any further suggestions. 81 Table 2 Instrument item literacy categories and item source Skill Source Phonological Awareness Break words intrgyllables Head Start Child Outcomes Framework Recognize rhymigggwords Head Start Child Outcomes Framework Phonemic Awareness Identify beginning sounds Kostelnik, Soderman, 8 Whiren (2004) Identify middle sounds Kostelnik, Soderman, 8 Whiren (2004) Identify end sounds Kostelnik, Soderman, 8 Whiren (2004) Alphabetic Principle Associate letter names with their sounds Michigan Department of Education Understand that letters are formed in a Michigan Department of Education specific way and are uncharyqi_ng Identify letters of the alphabet Head Start Child Outcomes Framework Oral Language Engage in conversation with peers with multiple exchanges Head Start Child Outcomes Framework Engage in conversation with adults with multiple exchagges Head Start Child Outcomes Framework Retell story details National Association for the Education of Young Children Pretend to read a book by telling a story fromthepicturesonthecoverorinthe book Kostelnik, Soderman, 8 Whiren (2004) Identify common objects (e.g., vehicles) Lansing School District Curriculum Benchmarks for Kindergarten Use descriptive language (e.g., I want to ay with the small, red truck) National Association for the Education of Young Children Respond to oral language with relevant comments and (mestions Michigan Department of Education Repeat songs, fingerplays, and movement activities High/Scope Child Observation Record Articulate intents, emotions, and desires Head Start Child Outcomes Framework Follow simple directions Head Start Child Outcomes Framework Formulate simple questions Kostelnik, Soderman, 8 Whiren (2004) Speak clearIy enough to be understood py others Head Start Child Outcomes Framework Use appropriate expressive vocabulary Michigan Department of Education Recognize common nouns and verbs typical of daily life Kostelnik, Soderman, 8 Whiren (2004) Retell a familiar story Kostelnik, Soderman, 8 Whiren (2004) Concepts of Print Use left-to-right, top-to—bottom orientation National Association for the Education of Young Children Recognize own name in writing Kostelnik, Soderman, 8 Whiren (2004) Understand where to begin reading Kostelnik, Soderman, 8 Whiren (2004) Identify title of a book Kostelnik, Soderman, 8 Whiren (2004) 82 Table 2 (cont’d). Understand that print contains a message Head Start Child Outcomes Framework Follow the print on a page, moving eyes in the correct direction High/Scope Child Observation Record Understand the difference between pictures and print Kostelnik, Soderman, 8 Whiren (2004) Develop a concept of punctuation Kostelnik, Soderman, 8 Whiren (2004) Understand the difference between letters and words Kostelnik, Soderman, 8 Whiren (2004) Recognize Iogographic print (e.g., McDonald’s, Kmart) Head Start Child Outcomes Framework Understand that words are separated by spaces Head Start Child Outcomes Framework Write letters of the alphabet High/Scope Child Observation Record Develop a sight vocabulary Micmgan Department of Education Write some words Michigan Department of Education Distinguish between real and make- believe Kostelnik, Soderman, 8 Whiren (2004) Write a sentence Michigan Department of Education Use invented/temporary spelIng Kostelnik, Soderman, 8 Whiren (2004) Exhibit reading-like behavior (e.g., turn pages correctly, moving from front to back) High/Scope Child Observation Record Write first name mghIScope Child Observation Record Write last name fighlScope Child Observation Record Draw a recognizable picture (self, family, objects) Kostelnik, Soderman, 8 Whiren (2004) Recognge lower case letters Michigan Department of Education Recpgnize upper case letters Michigan Department of Education Match spoken words with written ones National Association for the Education of Young Children Developmental Communication Abilities Listen attentively to stories Kostelnik, Soderman, 8 Whiren (2004) Select literacy experiences when given a choice National Association for the Education of Young Children Read others’ facial and body expressions Michigan State Board of Education Data Collection Procedure Upon committee approval of the proposal, approval was sought from Michigan State University’s Institutional Research Board, the University Committee on Research Involving Human Subjects (UCRIHS). In order to achieve UCRIHS approval for this study, a letter of consent to the kindergarten teachers was submitted (See Appendix C), and a letter of notification to the principals (See Appendix D), as well as assurances of complete confidentiality and privacy, and fulfillment of all other procedures as directed by UCRIHS. As soon as University approval was given see Appendix E), the researcher worked in cooperation with the IISD to distribute copies of the survey to the kindergarten teachers in Ingham County. Building principals at each school received a notification letter and a packet of surveys in individual envelopes via interdistrict mail. Teachers each received a copy of the survey along with a stamped return envelope via interdistrict mail, as well. Upon receiving the survey, kindergarten teachers had three weeks to fill it out and return it directly to the researcher. A reminder postcard was mailed to those teachers whose surveys had not been returned within the first two weeks. If there was difficulty achieving a 40 percent response rate, individual schools whose response rates are low were phoned to encourage the return of the surveys with additional surveys being sent via interdistrict mail if necessary. Teachers had an opportunity to participate in a drawing to win one of four $50.00 gift certificates to a local bookstore. As surveys were returned, the consent form with the teacher’s name, school name, and district on it was separated from the survey and put in a secure container. Once the deadline for the return of surveys had passed, four teachers names were drawn from the returned consent forms. The winners were contacted via public mail and sent one of the four gift certificates. Winners were from the Lansing (2), Mason (1), and Holt (1) School Districts. The information gathered from the surveys was kept in strict confidence. An identification code was given to each survey upon receipt in order better manage the data. However, the privacy of individual survey results was always protected. Only the researcher had access to the collected data. Anam Before data were entered into an SPSS program, a statistician double- checked the database set up to ensure accuracy and consistency. The analyses for this study looked at the differences in teacher perceptions, as well as the differences inherent in child situations, teacher situations, and contexts. For example, there are differences inherent in child situations such as SES and ethnicity. There are inherent differences in teacher situations such a length of employment and professional affiliation. As well, there are differences inherent in contexts such as all day or half-day kindergarten programs or low SES as compared to high SES contexts. This study also looked at the sources teachers believe have been most influential in forming their expectations for children’s readiness for kindergarten, along with whether teachers believed their expectations were congruent with other education specialists, perceptions, and standards. 85 A summary of the study questions, hypotheses, analysis methods for each question, and the survey questions specifically related to each hypothesis can be found in Table 3. The independent variables listed on the Emerging Literacy Skill Survey (See Appendix B) are purposefully unfactored. An alpha level of .05 was chosen, meaning that the probability of a type I error is 5 times (or less) in 100. Table 3 Summary of Research Questions, Hypotheses, and Analyses Research Question Hypothesis Survey Analysis question(s) corresponding to hypothesis 1. Which specific emerging 24 Frequencies, literacy skills do kindergarten Percentages teachers believe they are primarily responsible for teaching? 2. Is there any agreement 24 Frequencies, among kindergarten teachers Percentages as to which emerging literacy skills they believe should be primarily developed prior to kindergarten entrance? 3. Is there any agreement 24 Frequencies, among kindergarten teachers Percentages as to which emerging literacy skills they believe should be primarily developed prior to kindergarten entrance. 4. Is there any agreement 24 Frequencies, among kindergarten teachers Percentages as to which emerging literacy skills they believe should be primarily developed at the beginning of first grade? 87 Table 3 (cont’d). 5. Do perceptions of the acquisition of emerging literacy skills vary as a function of the demographics of kindergarten teachers? Hypothesis #5A There is no relationship between kindergarten teachers’ personal demographics and beliefs about the acquisition of emerging literacy skills 1,2 Frequencies, Crosstabs, Chi Square Hypothesis #58 There is no relationship between kindergarten teachers’ teaching background and beliefs about the acquisition of emerging literacy skills. 3,4,5 Frequencies, Crosstabs, Chi Square Hypothesis #50 There is no relationship between kindergarten teachers” education background and beliefs about the acquisition of emerging literacy skills. 6,7,8 Frequencies, Crosstabs, Chi Square Table 3 (cont'd). Hypothesis #50 There is no relationship between kindergarten teachers” participation in professional activities and beliefs about the acquisition of emerging literacy skills 9,10,11, 12 Frequencies, Crosstabs, Chi Square Hypothesis #5E There is no relationship between kindergarten teachers’ classroom characteristics and beliefs about the acquisition of emerging literacy skills 13,14,15, 16,17,18, 19 Frequencies, Crosstabs, Chi Square 6. What sources influence kindergarten teachers’ expectations of children’s literacy readiness? Hypothesis #6A There is no agreement among kindergarten teachers as to which sources most influence their expectations of children’s literacy readiness. 20 Frequencies, Crosstabs, Chi Square, Means 89 Table 3 (cont’d). 7. Are kindergarten teachers’ beliefs congruent with others regarding children’s literachapabilities? Hypothesis #7A 21 Frequencies, Crosstabs, There is no Chi Square, agreement among Means kindergarten teachers’ expectations and their perceived expectations of others regarding children’s literacy capabilities. 8. Which literacy skills do kindergarten teachers believe are the five most essential skills children should acquire by kindergarten entry? Hypothesis #8A 23 Frequencies, Crosstabs, There is no Chi Square agreement among kindergarten teachers as to which skills they think are the five most essential literacy skills children should acquire by kindergarten entry. Table 3 (cont’d). 9. Are kindergarten teacher expectations of skills to be developed during the kindergarten year congruent with the Stat of Michigan Kindergarten Framework of Skills? Hypothesis #9A 24 Percentages Kindergarten teacher expectations of skills to be developed during the kindergarten year are not congruent with the State of Michigan Framework of Skills. Limitations 1. The study was limited due to the reliability of the instrument. However, the internal consistency was tested by a pilot study. 2. The return rate of the surveys was less than 50 percent (actual rate of return was 32%). However, every effort was made to elicit the most returns possible. 3. There may have been differences between counties in Michigan that may mitigate generalizabilities to the state, as well as state differences between Michigan and other states in the United States. 4. As with other types of self-reported data, the data in this study may have differed from expressive behavior that is objectively measured. Therefore, the accuracy of this data may have been a limitation. 5. The unequal representation of ethnic groups of the respondents may have caused limitations of comparisons of certain demographics in this study (92.9% of respondents were White, 4.8% of respondents were African- American, and 2.4% of respondents were Hispanic). 91 Th an Summam This chapter reviewed the methods to be used in this study. The independent and dependent variables were described along with the population under study. A description of the instrument to be used was detailed along with a description of the data collection procedures. Nine research questions were proposed with corresponding hypotheses and the types of analyses to be used. Also noted were the limitations the study presented. A complete analysis of the data is presented in Chapter IV. 92 CHAPTER FOUR RESULTS This chapter presents demographic characteristics of the sample, followed by the results of the statistical analyses. These analyses are presented in order of the nine research questions. Results are presented in table form followed by an exploration of the findings. Frequencies were used to analyze research questions one through eight. Percentages were used in questions one, two, and nine. Crosstabs and Chi Square tests were run for questions five, six, and seven. Frequencies and percentages are reported in descending order of agreement. 93 Dempgraphic Characteristics of the Sample Demographic data were collected on the sample. Table 4 displays a comparison of the respondents on various demographic characteristics. Table 4 Comparison of Dempgraphics' LN=4;) Variable f % Age 21-30 yrs old 12 28.6 36-50 yrs old 15 35.7 51-65 yrs old 15 35.7 Ethnicity White 39 92.9 African-American 2 4.8 Hispanic 1 2.4 Years in Education 1-10 13 31.0 1 1-20 15 35.7 21-30 10 23.8 31-40 4 9.5 Years Teaching Kindergarten 1-5 12 28.6 6-10 18 42.9 1 1-35 12 28.6 Years Teaching Preschool 0 23 54.8 1-10 16 38.1 1 1-25 3 7.1 Major area of BA Study Elementary Education 20 47.6 Early Childhood 14 33.3 Other 8 19.0 All Cc Table 4 (cont’d) Variable f % Highest Degree Earned Bachelor 1 1 26.2 Master 29 69.0 Specialist 2 4.8 Early Childhood Endorsement No 21 50.0 Yes 21 50.0 Member of National Association for the Education of Young Children No 36 85.7 Yes 6 14.3 Subscribes to Professional Journals No 19 45.2 Yes 23 54.8 Attend Professional Conferences No 12 28.6 Yes 30 71.4 Conferences Attended Does not attend conferences 12 28.6 ACEI 1 2.4 MIAEYC 5 1 1.9 NAEYC 7 16.7 MRA 6 14.3 OTHER 1 1 26.2 Hours of Professional Development in the Last Year 0-15 8 19.0 16—39 10 23.8 40-60 14 33.3 61-200 9 21.4 Location of School Rural 4 9.5 Urban 38 90.5 95 Table 4 (cont’d). Variable f % Type of Program This Year ‘/2 day 16 38.1 all day, every day 24 57.1 all day, every other day 2 4.8 Number of Students in Classroom (Ave) 15-20 22 52.4 21-27 20 47.6 Age of Students in Classroom Age 5 0-9 students age 5 22 52.4 10-21 students age 5 18 42.9 Age 6 0-9 students age 6 20 47.6 10-23 students age 6 20 47.6 Age 7 0 students age 7 31 73.8 1-5 students age 7 10 23.8 Classroom Make-Up (%) African American children 0-10 16 38.1 1 1-25 7 16.7 26-50 1 1 26.2 >50 8 19.0 Asian children 0-10 37 88.1 1 1-25 4 9.5 26-50 1 2.4 Hispanic children 0-10 29 69.0 1 1-25 9 21.4 26-50 3 7.1 >50 1 2.4 White children 0-10 6 14.3 1 1-25 5 1 1.9 26-50 1 1 26.2 >50 20 47.6 1 1-25 2 4.8 Laps par hav lea Mo Ea we alt SU de st di: (4 Table 4 (cont’di. Variable f % Other ethnicities of children 0-10 40 95.2 Students Receiving Free/Reduced Lunch (%) 0-10 15 35.7 1 1-25 2 4.8 26-50 5 1 1.9 :59 20 47.6 Forty-two kindergarten teachers took part in this study. The ages of the participants were fairty evenly distributed as was the number of years teachers have taught kindergarten. Most teachers had between one and thirty years of teaching experience. The majority of teachers in this study were White (92.9%). Most teachers had obtained a Master Degree (69.0%) with half receiving an Early Childhood Endorsement. Interestingly, very few kindergarten teachers were members of the National Association for the Education of Young Children, although, in general, most do attend professional conferences (71.4%) and subscribe to professional journals (54.8%). The majority of kindergarten teachers in this study taught in an all day, every day program (57.1%). Classroom sizes ranged from 15 to 27 students with most students being 5 or 6 years of age. Ethnicity in classrooms was not often evenly distributed with many classrooms having either a majority of White students (47.6%) or pwdominately African-American (19.0%). About forty-seven percent of classrooms had at least half of the students receiving free or reduced lunch. 97 m Which specific emerging literacy skills do kindergarten teachers believe they are primarily responsible for teaching? Table 5 presents the list of emerging literacy skills kindergarten teachers were to consider in the survey. Results are listed in descending order of agreement. A majority indication will be where 50 percent or more of kindergarten teachers specified they were primarily responsible for teaching a certain emerging literacy skill. Also presented in this table are the data that indicate what percentage of kindergarten teachers supposed that their expectations for the acquisition of literacy skills were consistent with the perceived expectations of the school district in which they work. Table 5 Literacy Skills Teachers Believe They Are Primarily Responsible for Teaching (N=42) Emerging Is consistent with Literacy Sk_ill f % district demand Write some words 39 92.9 88.1 Understand the difference between letters and words 39 92.9 85.7 Associate letter names with their sounds 38 90.5 85.7 Understand that words are separated by spaces 38 90.5 83.3 Identify title of book 37 88.1 90.5 Identify beginning sounds 37 88.1 83.3 Write last name 36 85.7 88.1 Develop a sight vocabulary 36 85.7 90.5 Use Ieft-to—right orientation 35 83.3 83.3 Use invented/temporary spelling 35 83.3 85.7 Understand where to begin reading 35 83.3 81.0 Recognize rhyming words 35 83.3 85.7 Follow the print on a page moving eyes in the correct direction 34 81.0 85.7 Match spoken words with written ones 33 78.6 90.5 Understand that letters are formed in a specific way and are unchanging 33 78.6 83.3 Write a sentence 31 73.8 83.3 Retell story details 31 73.8 83.3 Recognize lower case letters 31 73.8 76.2 Retell a familiar story 29 69.0 90.5 Identify end sounds 29 69.0 78.6 Develop a concept of punctuation 28 66.7 81.0 Understand that print contains a message 27 64.3 76.2 Recognize upper case letters 27 64.3 78.6 Identify letters of the alphabet 25 59.5 76.2 Understand the difference between pictures and print 25 59.5 78.6 Formulate simple questions 25 59.5 81.0 Break words into syllables 23 54.8 88.1 Choose literacy experiences when given a choice 21 50.0 78.6 Respond to oral language with relevant comments and questions 19 45.2 78.6 Identify middle sounds 19 45.2 85.7 Recognize Iogographic print 18 42.9 81.0 Use descriptive language 17 40.5 71.4 Distinguish between real and make-believe 16 38.1 81 .0 Articulate intents, emotions, and desires 15 35.7 73.8 Write some letters 15 35.7 76.2 Table 5 (cont’dL Emerging ls consistent with Literacy 8ij f % tflsptrict demand Read others’ facial and body expressions 15 35.7 71.4 Use appropriate expressive language 12 28.6 73.8 Pretend to read a book by telling a story from the pictures 12 28.6 66.7 Engage in conversation with peers with multiple exchanges 12 28.6 76.2 Write first name 10 23.8 69.0 Draw a recognizable picture 10 23.8 64.3 Exhibit reading-like behavior 10 23.8 64.3 Recognize own name in writing 9 21.4 73.8 Recognize common nouns and verbs typical of everyday life 8 19.0 83.3 Listen attentively to stories 8 19.0 69.0 Identify common objects 8 19.0 71.4 Repeat songs, fingerplays and movement activities 7 16.7 64.3 Speak clearty enough to be understood by others 6 14.3 73.8 Engage in conversation with adults with multiple exchanges 3 7.1 71.4 Follow simple directions 3 7.1 69.0_ Of the fifty emerging literacy skills offered, the majority (over 50%) of kindergarten teachers indicated that they believe they are responsible for teaching twenty-eight of them. The majority of teachers also indicated that they thought their beliefs were consistent with district demand for those skills that they believed they are responsible for teaching, as well as for those skills they did not think they were responsible for teaching. 100 Question Two Is there any agreement among kindergarten teachers as to which emerging literacy skills they believe should be primarily developed prior to kindergarten entrance? Table 6 presents those emerging literacy skills for which a majority (50% or more) of kindergarten teachers agreed should be primarily developed prior to kindergarten entrance. Results are reported in descending order. 101 Table 6 Emerging Literacy Skills Kindergarten Teachers Believe Children Should Primariy Develop Prior to Kindergarten Entrance (N= 42) Emerging Literacy Skfll f % Engage in conversation with adults with multiple exchanges 39 92.9 Follow simple directions 39 92.9 Speak clearly enough to be understood by others 36 85.7 Repeat songs, fingerplays, movement activities 35 83.3 Listen attentively to stories 34 81.0 Identify common objects 34 81.0 Recognize own name in writing 33 78.6 Write first name 32 76.2 Exhibit reading like behavior telling a story from the picture 32 76.2 Pretend to read a book by telling a story from the pictures 30 71.4 Write some letters of the alphabet 27 64.3 Draw a recognizable picture 26 61.9 Distinguish between real and make believe 26 61.9 Articulate intents, emotions, and desires 25 59.5 Engage in conversation with peers with multiple exchanges 25 59.5 Use appropriate expressive vocabulary 24 57.1 Recognize Iogographic print 22 52.4 Uses descriptive language 22 52.4 Read others facial and body expressions 22 52.4 Respond to oral language with relevant comments, Questions 21 50.0 102 Of the 50 emerging literacy skills offered, kindergarten teachers indicated that they thought 20 of them should be primarily developed prior to kindergarten entrance. Two-thirds of the teachers agreed that 8 of these skills should be developed prior to kindergarten entrance. At least half of all teachers agreed the remaining 12 skills should be developed prior to kindergarten entrance. Ten of these skills were oral language skills, 8 were concept of print skills, and 2 were developmental communication skills. Question Three Is there any agreement among kindergarten teachers as to which emerging literacy skills they believe should be primarily developed after kindergarten entrance? Table 7 presents those emerging literacy skills for which a majority (50% or more) of kindergarten teachers agreed should be primarily developed after kindergarten entrance. 103 Table 7 Emerging Literacy Skills Teachers Believe Should Be Primarily Developed After Kindergarten Entrance (N=42) Emerging Literag Skill f % Write some words 39 92.9 Understand the difference between letters and words 39 92.9 Associate letter names with their sounds 38 90.5 Understand that words are separated 38 90.5 by spaces Identify title of book 37 88.1 Identify beginning sounds 37 88.1 Write last name 36 85.7 Develop a sight vocabulary 36 85.7 Use left-to-right orientation 35 83.3 Use invented/temporary spelling 35 83.3 Understand where to begin reading 35 83.3 Recognize rhyming words 35 83.3 Follow the print on a page moving eyes in the correct direction 34 81.0 Match spoken words with written ones 33 78.6 Understand that letters are formed in a specific way and are unchanging 33 78.6 Write a sentence 31 73.8 Retell story details 31 73.8 Recognize lower case letters 31 73.8 Retell a familiar story 29 69.0 Identify and sounds 29 69.0 Develop a concept of punctuation 28 66.7 Understand that print contains a message 27 64.3 Recognize upper case letters 27 64.3 Identify letters of the alphabet 25 59.5 Understand the difference between pictures and print 25 59.5 Formulate simple questions 25 59.5 Break words into syllables 23 54.8 Choose literacy experiences when given a choice 21 5g.0 104 Of the 50 emerging literacy skills offered, kindergarten teachers specified that 28 of the skills should be developed after kindergarten entrance. Two-thirds of the teachers agreed on 15 of these skills, whereas at least 50 percent of teachers agreed that the remaining thirteen emerging literacy skills should be developed after kindergarten entrance. Of those skills, 17 are concept of print skills, 3 are oral language skills, 3 are alphabetic principle skills, 2 are phonological awareness skills, 2 are phonemic awareness skills, and 1 is a developmental communication skill. Question Four Is there any agreement among kindergarten teachers as to which emerging literacy skills they believe should be primarily developed during the first grade? Table 8 presents those emerging literacy skills for which a majority (50% or more) of kindergarten teachers agreed should be primarily developed during early first grade. 105 Table 8 . Emerging Literacy Skills Kindergarten Teachers Believe Should Be Primarily Develpfl Duripg Ear_ly First Grade (N= 4g) Emerging Literacy Skill f % Identify middle sounds 23 54.8 Recognize common nouns and verbs typical of daiy life 22 5_2_.4 Of the 50 emerging literacy skills listed, over 50 percent of kindergarten teachers agreed that only 2 of them should be primarily be developed during early first grade. One of the skills was a phonemic awareness skill and the other was an oral language skill. Question Five Do perceptions of the acquisition of emerging literacy skills vary as a function of the demographics of kindergarten teachers? Table 9 gives a Chi-Square summary of the acquisition of emerging literacy skills as a function of teaching background. 106 Table 9 Chi-Square Summary of Perceptions of the Acquisition of Emerging Literacy Skills as a Function of Teachipg Background (N= 4;) Variable 91‘ X2 P__ Years in education and Use inventedltemporary spelling 6 12.597 .050 Years in education and Understand that words are separated by spaces 6 15.418 .017 Years in education and Write a sentence 6 14.865 .012 Years teaching preschool and Identity title of book 4 17.607 .001b Years teaching preschool and Understand where to begin reading 2 8.713 .013 Years teaching preschool and Follow print on page, moving eyes in the correct direction 4 14.596 .006 Years teaching preschool and Writes some letters of the alphabet 4 17.607 .0Q1_ Significant experience differences were found regarding how many years in education and use invented/temporary spelling [X2](6, N=42)= 12.597, p=.050. Alter kindergarten entrance had a higher than expected count by teachers with 1- 10 years in education and a lower than expected count by teachers with 21-30 years in education. During earty first grade a higher than expected count by teachers with 21-30 years in education. Significant experience differences were found regarding years in education and understand that words are separated by spaces [X2](6, N=42)= 15.418, p=.017. After kindergarten entrance had a higher than expected count by 107 teachers with 1-10 years in education and 21-30 years in education, and a lower than expected count by teachers with 11-20 years in education. During early first grade had a higher than expected count by teachers with 11-20 years in education. Significant experience differences were found in terms of years in education and write a sentence [X2](6, N=42)= 14.865, p=.012. After kindergarten entrance had a higher than expected count by teachers with 1-10 years in education and a lower than expected count by teachers with 21-30 years in education. During early first grade had a higher than expected count by teachers with 21-30 years in education and a lower than expected count by teachers with 1-10 and 31-40 years in education. As shown in Table 9, significant differences were found in terms of years teaching preschool and identify title ofa book [x21(4, N=42)= 17.607, p=.001. After kindergarten entrance had a lower than expected count by teachers with 11-25 years of preschool teaching. During early first grade had a lower than expected count by teachers with 1-10 years of preschool teaching experience. Significant differences were found in terms of years teaching preschool and understand where to begin reading [X2] (2, N=42)= 8.713, p=.013. Fewer than expected teachers with no preschool teaching experience said prior to kindergarten, while more than the expected amount of teachers with 1-10 years of preschool teaching and kindergarten teachers with 11-25 years of preschool teaching experience said prior to kindergarten. As could be expected then, more than the expected number of kindergarten teachers with no preschool 108 teaching experience said after kindergarten entrance and fewer than expected kindergarten teachers with 11-25 years of preschool teaching experience said the same. As seen in Table 9, significant differences were found regarding years teaching preschool and follow print on page, moving eyes in the correct direction [x21(4, N=42)= 14.596, p=.006. Prior to kindergarten a higher than expected count by teachers with 1-10 years of preschool teaching. After kindergarten entrance had a higher than expected count by teachers with no prescth teaching experience and a lower than expected count by teachers with 1-10 years of preschool teaching experience. Significant differences were found regarding years teaching preschool and writes some letters [x2] (4, N=42)= 17.607, p=.001. Prior to kindergarten had a lower than expected count by kindergarten teachers with no preschool teaching experience and a higher than expected count by teachers with 1-10 years and 11-25 years of preschool teaching experience. After kindergarten entrance had a higher than expected count by kindergarten teachers with no preschool teaching experience and a lower than expected count by kindergarten teachers with 1-10 and 11-25 years of preschool teaching experience. Table 10 presents a Chi-square summary of the perceptions of the acquisition of emerging literacy skills as a function of kindergarten teachers’ education background. 109 Table 10 Chi-Square Summary of Perceptions of the Acquisition of Emerging Literacy Skills as a Function of Education Background (N= 43L Variable at x2 p Major area of BA and Listen attentively to stories 2 13.140 .001 Highest degree earned and Follow simple directions 2 9.790 .007 Highest degree earned and Use invented/temporary spelling 4 20.706 .000 Highest degree earned and Understand that words are separated by spaces 4 20.588 .000 Highest degree earned and Develop a concept of punctuation 4 10.161 .038 Highest degree earned and Write a sentence 4 21.053 .000 Highest degree earned and Identify middle sounds 2 6.164 .046 Early childhood endorsement and Retell story details 2 8.251 .016 Early childhood endorsement and Listen attentively to stories 1 9.882 .002 Early childhood endorsement and Articulate intents emotions and desires 2 L507 .014 —4— Three areas of teachers’ education background were significant: major area of BA, highest degree earned, and early childhood endorsement. Significant differences were found in regard to major area of BA and listen attentively to stories, [X2](2, N=42)= 13.140, p=.001. Prior to kindergarten had a higher than expected count by kindergarten teachers whose major area of study was elementary education and “other” area, while a lower than expected count by those kindergarten teachers with an early childhood major was found. After kindergarten entrance had a higher than expected count by early childhood majors and a lower than expected count by elementary education and “other” majors. Significant differences were found regarding highest degree earned and follow simple directions [X2](2, N=42)=9.790, p=.007. Prior to kindergarten had a higher than expected count by kindergarten teachers who had earned a Master’s degree and a lower than expected count by kindergarten teachers whose highest degree was a Bachelor’s degree. Alter kindergarten entrance had a higher than expected count by kindergarten teachers with a Bachelor's degree and a lower than expected count by kindergarten teachers with a Master’s degree. One significant difference was found with highest degree and write a sentence [x2](4, N=42)= 21.053, p= .000. During early first grade had a gmater than expected count by kindergarten teachers with a Master's degree. Significant differences were discovered concerning highest degree earned and identify middle sounds [x2](2, N=42)= 6.164, p=.046. Alter kindergarten entrance had a greater than expected count by kindergarten teachers with a Master’s degree and a lower than expected count by kindergarten teachers with a Bachelor‘s degree. During early first grade had a higher than expected count by teachers with a Bachelor’s degree and a lower than expected count by teachers with a Master’s degree. 111 Significant differences were found as regards highest degree eamed and use invented/temporary spelling [X2](4, N=42)=20.706, p=.000, understand that words are separated by spaces [x2](4, N=42)=20.588, p=.000, and develop a concept of punctuation [X2](4, N=42)= 10.161, p=.038. However, no clear pattern of distribution emerged. As shown in Table 10, significant differences were found concerning early childhood endorsement and retell story details [X2](2, N=42)= 8.251, p=.016, articulate intents, emotions, and desires [X2](2, N=42)=8.507, p=.014, and listen attentively to stories [X2](2, N=42)=9.882, p=.002. In all of these cases: prior to kindergarten had higher expected counts by kindergarten teachers who did not have an early childhood endorsement and lower than expected counts by kindergarten teachers who did have an early childhood endorsement; alter kindergarten entrance had higher than expected counts by kindergarten teachers who did have an early childhood endorsement and lower than expected counts by those who did not Table 11 shows a Chi-square summary of perceptions of the acquisition of emerging literacy skills as a function of kindergarten teachers’ participation in professional activities. 112 Table 11 Chi-Square Summary of Perceptions of the Acquisition of Emerging Literacy Skills as a Function of Teacher Partici ation in Professional Activities N= 42 Variable gr x2 p Member of NAEYC and Follow print on page, moving eyes in the correct direction 2 7.172 .028 Subscribe to professional journals and Identify middle sounds 1 4.497 .034 Attend professional conferences and Exhibit reading-like behavior 1 6.353 .012 Conferences attended and Recognize lower case letters 5 1 1.174 .048 Conferences attended and Understand the difference between pictures and print 5 16.490 .006 Hours of professional development and Rmnize upgr case letters 3 Q8076 p055 Four areas of teacher participation in professional activities were significant: member of NAEYC, subscribe to professional joumals, attend professional conferences, and hours of professional development. A significant difference was found regarding membership in the Nafional Association for the Education of Young Children (NAEYC) and follow print on page, moving eyes in the correct direction [X2](2,N=42)=7.172, p=.028. Those teachers who were not NAEYC members had a higher than expected count in prior to kindergarten, while those kindergarten teachers who were members of NAEYC had a lower than expected count. 113 Significant differences were found concerning subscribe to professional journals and identify middle sounds [x2](1, N=42)=4.497, p=.034. Teachers who did not subscribe to professional journals had a higher than expected count in alter kindergarten entrance and a lower than expected count in during early first grade, while teachers who did subscribe to professional journals had a lower than expected count in after kindergarten entrance and a higher than expected count in during early first grade. Significant differences were found regarding attend professional conferences and exhibit reading like behavior [X2](1, N42)=6.353, p=.012. Kindergarten teachers who do not attend professional conferences had a lower than expected count and those who did attend conferences had a higher than expected count in prior to kindergarten, whereas the opposite was true in after kindergarten entrance. Significant differences were found related to conferences attended and: recognize lower case letters [x2](5, N=42)=11.174, p=.048. Counts in pridr to kindergarten were higher than expected in teachers who do not attend conferences and lower than expected in teachers who attend NAEYC and “other” conferences. Counts in after kindergarten entrance were higher than expected in teachers who attend NAEYC and “other” conferences and lower in teachers who do not attend professional conferences. Significant differences were also found related to conferences attended and understand the difference between pictures and print [X2](5, N=42)=16.490, p=.006. Counts in prior to kindergarten were higher than expected by those who 114 attend NAEYC and MRA conferences and lower than expected by those who do attend MIAEYC and “other“ conferences, and by those who do not attend professional conferences. Counts in after kindergarten entrance were lower than expected by those who attend MRA and NAEYC conferences and higher than expected by those who attend MIAEYC, “other” conferences, and by those who do not attend conferences. In the chi square test, significant differences were found concerning hours of development and recognize upper case letters [X2])3, N=42)=8.076, p=.044. Prior to kindergarten counts were higher than expected for teachers with 40—60 hours of professional development last year and lower than expected for teachers with 61-200 hours of professional development last year. Counts in after kindergarten enhance were the reverse. Table 12 shows a Chi-square summary of perceptions of the acquisition of emerging literacy skills as a function of teachers’ classroom characteristies. 115 Table 12 Chi-Square Summary of Perceptions of the Acquisition of Emerging Literacy Skills as a Function of Classroom Characteristies (N= 4_2_) Variable or x2 9 Type of program this year and Distinguish between real and make—believe 2 6.398 .041 Type of program this year and Write first name 2 7.547 .023 Type of program this year and Write last name 4 20.562 .000 Number of students in classroom and Write first name 1 4.041 .045 Number of students in classroom and Draw a recognizable picture 1 4.627 .031 Number of students in classroom and Responds to oral language with relevant comments and questions 2 6.355 .042 Number of students in classroom and Retell story details 2 7.657 .022 Number of students in classroom and Understand that letters are formed in a specific way and are unchanging 2 7.740 .021 Number of children 5 years old and _ Writes some letters of the alphabet 1 8.210 .004 Number of children 5 years old and Exhibit reading-like behavior 1 4.268 .039 Number of children 5 years old and Break words into syllables 1 6.861 .009 116 Table 12 (cont’d). Variable x2 9 Number of children 5 years old and Recognize own name in writing 9.501 .002 Number of children 6 years old and Exhibit reading-like behavior 10.000 .002 Number of children 7 years old and Retell story details 6.536 .038 Percent of students who are African-American and Distinguish between real and make-believe 13.223 .004 Percent of students who are Asian and Understand that letters are formed in a specific way and are unchanging 14.344 .006 Percent of students who are Asian and Follow print on page, moving eyes in the correct direction 15.477 .004” Percent of students who are Asian and Identify end sounds 14.962 .005 Percent of students who are Asian and Identify beginning sounds 10.140 .038 Percent of students who are Hispanic and Recognize rhyming words 13.980 .030 Percent of students who are Hispanic and Retell story details 20.941 .002 Percent of students who are Hispanic and Identify end sounds 14.966 .021 Percent of students who are White and Recognize Iogographic print 7.842 .049 117 Table 12 (cont'd). Variable Percent of students receiving free and reduced lunch and Write some letters of the alphabet Percent of students with childcare experience and Write last name Percent of students with developmental kindergarten experience and Write some words Percent of students with developmental kindergarten experience and Engage in conversation with adults with multiple exchanges Percent of students with developmental kindergarten experience and Recognize common nouns and verbs typical of everyday life Percent of students with developmental kindergarten experience and Write some words Percent of students with Head Start or MSRP experience and Formulate simple questions Percent of students with private preschool experience and Recognize I0909I’aphic print Percent of students with private preschool experience and Identity middle sounds 118 '9'. x2 9.406 13.607 1 1.939 8.947 11.505 1 1.939 16.864 1 1.820 7.946 .024 .034 .018 .011 .021 .018 .032 .008 .047 Table 12 (cont’d). Variable Percent of students with private preschool experience and Understand the difference between letters 6 16.389 .012 and words '9. ZS. L. Percent of students with no preschool experience and MW mmm om (e.g. vehicles) 4 1_2_.977 .011 Thirty-seven significant variables are noted as a function of 16 different classroom characteristies. As seen in Table 12, significant differences were discovered regarding the type of program the teacher taught and distinguish between real and make-believe [X2](2, N=42)=6.398, p=.041, write first name [X2](2, N=42)=7.547, p=.023, and write last name [X2](4, Na2)=20.562, p=.000. Distinguish between real and make-believe had a higher than expected count in prior to kindergarten entrance by teachers who taught a ‘/2 day program as compared to teacher who taught in an all day, every day program who had a lower than expected count. Conversely, the counts for alter kindergarten entrance were lower than expected for teachers in a ‘/2 day program and higher than expected for teachers in an all day, every day program. Counts for prior to kindergarten entrance and write first name were higher than expected for teachers in a ‘A day program and lower than expected by teachers in all day, every other day programs. The counts for after kindergarten entrance were lower than expected in the V2 day program and higher than expected in the all 119 day, every other day program. While significance is noted for program type and write last name, no clear tendency in the distribution of the variable was noted. Significant differences were found concerning number of students in the classroom and write first name [X2](1, N=42)=4.014, p=.045. Prior to kindergarten had a higher than expected count in classrooms with 21-27 students whereas a lower than expected count was found in classrooms with 15- 20 students. The reverse was found for after kindergarten entrance. The number of students in the classroom was found to be significant regarding respond to oral language with relevant comments and questions [X2](2, N=42)=6.355, p=.042. The prior to la’ndergarten count in classrooms with 15-20 students was higher than expected while the count in the 21-27 student classroom was lower than expected. Again, the opposite counts were found with the after kindergarten entrance variable. As shown in Table 12, the number of students in the classroom is significant in terms of retell story details [X2](2, N=42)=7.657, p=.022. Classrooms with 15- 20 students were significantly overrepresented while classrooms with 21-27 students were significantly underrepresented prior to kindergarten enby. After kindergarten entrance counts were lower than expected in classrooms with 15—20 students and overrepresented in classrooms with 21-27 students. The counts in during early first grade were underrepresented for both sizes of classrooms. Significance was also found for number of students in classroom and understand that letters are formed in a specific and unchanging way [X2](2, N=42)=7.740, p=.021. Counts in after kindergarten entrance were higher than 120 expected in classrooms with 15—20 students and lower than expected in classrooms with 21-27 students. Counts in during early first grade were significantly lower than expected in classrooms with 15—20 students and significantly higher in classrooms with 21-27 students. Significant differences were uncovered for number of children 5-years-old and write some letters of the alphabet [x2](1, N=42)=8.210, p=.004, exhibit reading- Iike behavior [X2](1, N=42)=4.286, p=.039, and recognize own name in writing, [X2](1, N=42)=9.501, p=.002. Prior to kindergarten counts for each variable were significantly lower than emcted in classrooms with 0-9 students at age 5 and significantly higher in classrooms with 10—21 students age 5. After kindergarten entrance counts were the reverse for each. Significant findings for number of children 5-years-olds and; break words into syllables [X2](1, N=42)=6.861, p=.009 note that counts for alter kindergarten entrance are significantly lower than expected in classrooms with 0-9 students age 5 and significantly higher for classrooms with 10-21 students age 5. Counts for during early first grade were the opposite. Significant differences were found as regards the number of 6- year-olds in the classroom and exhibit reading-like behavior [x2](1, N=40)=10.001, p=.002. Significantly higher counts than expected in prior to kindergarten were found in classrooms with 0-9 children 6-years-old and significantly lower than expected in classrooms with 10—23 students 6-years-old. The counts in after kindergarten entrance were found to be the reverse. One significant finding was noted for classrooms with children 7-years-old, retell story details [x2](2, N=41)=6.536, p=.038. The count in alter kindergarten entrance 121 was overrepresented in classrooms where there were no children 7-years-old and underrepresented in classrooms with 1-5 children who were 7-years-old. The reverse held true for during early first grade. As shown in Table 12, significant differences are found in the percentage of students who are African-American and distinguish between real and make- believe [X2](3, Na2)=13.223, p=.004. Discrepancies in expected count and actual count in prior to kindergarten include a higher than expected count in classrooms where 0—10 percent of students are African-American and lower than expected counts in classrooms that were 11-25 percent and 26-50 percent African-American. Conversely, in alter kindergarten entrance there is a significantly lower expected count in classrooms that were 0—10 percent African- American and significantly higher counts in classrooms that were 11-25 and 26— 50 percent African-American. Chi-square testing uncovered significant differences in percent of students who are Asian and identify end sounds [X2](4, N=42)=14.962, p=.005. The alter kindergarten entrance count for classrooms with 0-10 percent of students“... , . are Asian was higher than expected while there was also a higher than expected count during first grade in classrooms with 11-25 percent of students who are Asian. Significant findings were also seen regarding percent of students who are Asian and follow print on page, moving eyes in the correct direction [X2](4, N=42)=15.477, p=.004. Counts were found to be higher than expected in alter kindergarten entrance in classrooms with 0-10 percent of students who are Asian. Prior to kindergarten entrance had lower than expected counts in 122 classrooms with 0-10 and higher than expected counts in classrooms with 11-25 percent of students who are Asian. Two other significant findings regarding percent of students who are Asian, identify beginning sounds [X2](4, N=42)=10.140, p=.038 and understand that letters are formed in a specific and unchanging way [X2](4, N=42)=14.344, p=.006, gave no obvious tendency of distribution. Significant differences were found in classrooms wifl'l Hispanic students and identify and sounds [x21(4, N=42)=14.966, p=.021. The count in prior to kindergarten in classrooms with 0-10 percent Hispanic students was lower than expected. Alter kindergarten entrance had a lower than expected in count in classrooms with 11-25 percent of students who are Hispanic. During early first grade found a higher than expected count in classrooms with 0-10 percent of students who are Hispanic. Two other variables, retell story details [X2](6, N=42)=20.941, p=.002 and recognize rhyming words [X2](6, N=42)=13.980, p=.030, while significant showed no evident tendency in the distribution. The study analyses showed a significance in the percent of students who are White and recognize Iogographic print [x2](3, N=42)=7.842, p=.049. Prior to kindergarten had higher than expected counts in classrooms with 0-10, 11-25, and 26-50 percent of students who are White. A lower than expected count was found in classrooms with more than 50 percent of students who are White. Alter la'ndergarten entrance had lower than expected counts in 0-10, 11-25, and 26-50 percent of students who are White, and a higher than expected count in classrooms with more than 50 percent of students who are White. 123 Significant differences were found in the percent of students receiving free/reduced lunch and write some letters of the alphabet [x2](3, N=42)=9.406, p=.024. Priortc kindergarten had higher than expected counts in classrooms with 26—50 and more than 50 percent of students receiving free/reduced lunch, and lower than expected counts in classrooms with 0—10 and 11-25 percent of students receiving freelmduced lunch. Alter kindergarten entrance had inverse findings. Significant differences were uncovered in relation to students and program experiences before kindergarten entry. The skill, write last name [X2](6, N=41)=13.607, p=.034) showed higher than expected counts by teachers who had 51-75 percent of students with childcare experience. There appeared to be significant differences in perspective for teachers with children who had developmental kindergarten experience and engage in conversation with adults with multiple exchanges [x2](2, N=42)=8.947, p=.011). Teachers with no children coming from developmental kindergarten had a higher than expected count in prior to kindergarten. Teachers with 1-25 percent of students with developmental kindergarten experience had a higher than expected count in alter kindergarten entrance. Significant differences were noted for teachers with students with developmental kindergarten experience and recognize common nouns and verbs typical of daily life [X2](4, N=42)=11.505, p=.021). Teachers with 1-25 percent of students with developmental kindergarten experience showed a higher than expected count in prior to kindergarten and a lower than expected count in alter kindergarten entrance and during early first grade. Teachers with students who 124 had no students with developmental kindergarten experience had lower than expected counts in prior to kindergarten and alter kindergarten entrance and a higher than expected count in during early first grade. Teacher with 26-50 percent of students with developmental kindergarten experience had a higher than expected count in during early first grade. Teacher perceptions were found to be significant in regard to the concept of print skill, write some words [X2](4, N=42)=11.939, p=.018), however no clear tendency of distribution is noted. A significant teacher perspective differences was found regarding the percent of students with Head Start or Michigan School Readiness Program (MSRP) experience and formulate simple questions [X2](8, N=42)=16.864, p=.032). Teacher with no students experiencing Head Start or MSRP programs had a higher than expected count in prior to kindergarten and a lower than expected count in alter kindergarten entrance. Teachers with 1-25 percent of students with Head Start or MSRP program experience had a higher expected count in after kindergarten entrance and a lower than expected count in prior to kindergarten. A significant difference was found concerning teaches who had students with private preschool experience and recognize Iogographic print [X2](3,N=42)=11.820, p=.008). Teachers with no children having private preschool experience had a higher than expected count in prior to kindergarten and a lower than expected count in alter kindergarten entrance. Teachers with 26-50 percent of students with private preschool experience showed a higher than expected count in alter kindergarten entrance and a lower that expected count in prior to la'ndergarten. Those teachers with 51-75 percent of students 125 with private preschool experience had a higher than expected count in alter kindergarten entrance and a lower than expected count in prior to kindergarten. A similar pattern is found for teacher perceptions with students who had private preschool experience and identify middle sounds [x2](3, N=42)=7.946, p=.047). Kindergarten teachers who had no children with private preschool experience had a higher than expected count in alter kindergarten entrance and a lower than expected count in during early first grade. Teachers with from 1-75 percent of students who had private preschool experience had higher than expected counts in during early first grade and lower than expected counts in after kindergarten entrance. Another point of significance for kindergarten teacher perceptions when looking at the percent of students with private preschool experience is understand the dilterence between letters and words [X2](6, N=42)=16.389, p=.012. Teachers who had no children with private preschool experience had a higher than expected count in alter kindergarten entrance and a lower than expected count in prior to kindergarten. Teachers with 51-75 percent of children with private preschool experience had a higher than expected count in prior to kindergarten and a lower than expected count in alter kindergarten entrance. A significant difference in teacher perceptions regarding the percent of students with no preschool experience and identify common objects [x2](4, N=42)=12.977, p=.011) was found. Teachers with students who had either no preschool experience or teachers with 1-25 percent of students with no preschool experience had higher than expected counts in prior to kindergarten and lower than expected counts in alter kindergarten entrance. Teachers with 26-50 and 126 51-75 percent of students with no preschool experience had higher than expected counts in after kindergarten entrance and lower than expected counts in prior to kindergarten. uestion Six What sources influence kindergarten teachers’ expectations of children's literacy readiness for kindergarten? Table 13 shows the sources of influence, the frequency and percent of agreement for each degree of influence, as well as, in descending order, the mean score for each source as to the degree of influence it has on teacher expectations (where, 1= not at all influential, 3= somewhat influential, and 5= very influential). 127 Table 13 Influential Sources on Kindergarten Teacher Expectations of Children’s Literacy Readiness (N= 42) Source f % m Past teaching experience 4.40 .86 2 1 2.4 3 7 16.7 4 8 19.0 5 26 61.9 Professional conferences 4.29 .86 1 1 2.4 2 1 2.4 3 2 4.8 4 19 45.2 5 19 45.2 School district curriculum 4.17 .91 2 1 2.4 3 1 1 26.2 4 10 23.8 5 20 47.6 State curriculum framework 4.02 .78 3 12 28.6 4 17 40.5 5 13 31.0 Other kindergarten teachers 4.00 .99 1 1 2.4 2 1 2.4 3 1 1 26.2 4 13 31.0 5 16 38.1 ln-service training 4.00 .86 2 2 4.8 3 9 21.4 4 18 42.9 5 13 31.0 Expectations of first grade teachers 3.60 .96 1 1 2.4 2 4 9.5 3 13 31.0 4 17 40.5 5 7 16.7 128 SD Table 13 (cont’d). Source f % in SD Assessment used by the district 3.33 1.26 1 5 11.9 2 5 11.9 3 11 26.2 4 13 31.0 5 8 19.0 Professional journals 3.31 1.22 1 7 16.7 3 13 31.0 4 17 40.5 5 5 11.9 Federal legislation 3.05 1.41 1 8 19.0 2 7 16.7 3 11 26.2 4 7 16.7 5 9 21.4 How I was taught as a child 2.21 1.30 1 17 40.5 2 10 23.8 3 7 16.7 4 5 11.9 5 3 7.1 Local school board 2.21 1.22 1 17 40.5 2 7 16.7 3 12 28.6 4 4 9.5 5 2 4.8 Media sources 1.76 2.00 1 20 47.6 2 14 33.3 3 7 16.7 5 1 2.4 Of the 13 possible sources of influence, kindergarten teachers indicated 10 of them are somewhat influential to very influential. The most influential source indicated was past teaching experience with about 62 percent of teachers 129 agreeing that it was very influential (5, m= 4.40) on their expectations of children’s literacy readiness. Media sources was the least influential with about 48 percent of teachers agreeing that they are not influential at all (1, m= 1.76). While the data did not reflect any other points of majority agreement, teachers indicated that they were also quite influenced by professional conferences (m= 4.29), school district cuniculum (4.17), the state curriculum framework (4.02), other kindergarten teachers (4.00), and in-service training (4.00). Interestingly, teachers were strongly divided in their reports of the influence of federal legislation as about 36% of teachers said that federal legislation is not at all influential on their expectations. In contrast, 38% of teachers indicated that federal legislation was very influential on their expectations (m= 3.05). About 41 percent of teachers said that the local school board was not at all influential, whereas about 30 percent noted that it is somewhat influential. Table 14 gives a Chi-square summary of the most influential sources of kindergarten teachers’ expectations of children’s literacy readiness for kindergarten. Emerging literacy skills were grouped by type: oral language, concept of print, phonological awareness, phonemic awareness, alphabetic principle, and developmental communication. Scores of respondents were then added and assigned an overall score for analysis. Only significant results at the 5% and 1% level are presented. 130 Table 14 Chi-Square Summary of The Most Influential Sources of Kindergarten Teachers’ Whom of Children’s Literag Readiness for Kindergarten (N= 42) Variable it x2 9 Past teaching experiences . Concept of print skills 51 71.360 .031 Past teaching experience Phonolggical awareness skills 9 43.675 .000 Past teaching experience was found to be the only influential source of teacher expectations that was significant. While significance was found regarding concept of print differences [x2](51, N=42)=71.360, p=.031 and phonological awareness [x21(9, N=42)=43.675, p=.ooo and past teaching experience, no obvious distribution tendency was found. 9mm Are kindergarten teachers’ beliefs congruent with others’ beliefs regarding children’s literacy capabilities? Table 15 notes the degree to which teachers think that their beliefs about children’s literacy capabilities are congruent with preschool teachers’ expectations, other kindergarten teachers’ expectations, first grade teachers’ expectations, parents’ expectations, standards set by flleir school district, the state curriculum framework, and the standards set by Federal legislation (where, 1= not at all congruent, 3= somewhat congruent, and 5= highly congruent; non- marked values were left off). This table also indicates the mean score for each variable. 131 Table 15 Congmency of Kindergarten Teacher Beliefs of Children’s Literacy Readiness with Others (N=41) Source f % m §l_I_)_ Other kindergarten teachers 4.12 .95 1 1 2.4 2 1 2.4 3 7 17.1 4 15 36.6 5 17 41.5 First grade teachers 3.90 .77 2 1 2.4 3 1 1 26.8 4 20 48.8 5 9 21.4 Standards set by my school district 3.85 .88 2 2 4.9 3 13 31.7 4 15 36.6 5 1 1 26.8 State curriculum framework 3.80 .98 2 4 9.8 3 12 29.3 4 13 31.7 5 12 29.3 Parents’ expectation 3.51 .87 1 1 2.4 2 3 7.3 3 15 36.6 4 18 43.9 5 5 9.8 Standards set by Federal legislation* 3.41 1.00 1 1 9.5 2 6 14.3 3 15 36.6 4 13 31 .7 5 6 14.6 Preschool teacher expectations 3.39 .95 1 1 2.4 2 6 14.6 3 14 34.1 4 16 39.0 5 4 9.8 *Standards may be based on previous set of expectationskor No Child Left Behind Standards of 2000. 132 Kindergarten teachers indicated that they think that their beliefs about children’s literacy capabilities are most congruent with other kindergarten teachers and least congruent with preschool teachers. Notably, there was no majority of agreement regarding the degree of congruency for any of the choices among kindergarten teachers. However, about 77 percent of teachers did indicate that they thought their beliefs were congruent (4) to highly congruent (5) with first grade teachers. More than half of all kindergarten teachers said that their beliefs are congruent (4) to highly congruent (5) with standards set by my school district (63.4%), state curriculum framework (61%), and parent expectations (53.7%). Table 16 shows a Chi-square summary of kindergarten teachers’ expectations and their perceptions of the expectations of others. Emerging literacy skills were grouped by type: oral language, concept of print, phonological awareness, phonemic awareness, alphabetic principle, and developmental communication. Scores of respondents were then added and assigned an overall score for analysis. Only significant results at the 5% and 1% level are presented. 133 Table 16 Chi-Square Summary of Kindergarten Teachers’ Expectations and Their Perceived ExmtionsLofj Others (N= 42) Variable or x2 9 Standards set by my school district Oral laggugge skills 412 62.830 .020 Significant differences were only found in regard to oral language differences and standards set by my scth district [X2](42, N=41)=62.830. There was no distribution tendency noted. Question Eight Which literacy skills do kindergarten teachers believe are the five most essential skills children should acquire by kindergarten entry? Table 17 indicates those skills kindergarten teachers believed to be top five emerging literacy skills children should have upon kindergarten entry. The skills are listed in descending order. There were 197 hits out of a possible 250. 134 Table 17 Most Important Emerging Literacy Skills Children Should Have Upon Kimggten Entrv (N= 42) (Total count= 197) I_.'rt_e_r§_c3LSki_ll f % of mismnses Follow simple directions 20 10.2 Listen attentively to stories 18 9.1 Repeat songs, fingerplays, and movement activities 18 9.1 Exhibit reading4ike behavior 12 6.1 Identify letters of the alphabet 12 6.1 Engage in conversations with peers/adults 11 5.6 Draw a recognizable picture 10 5.1 Write first name 9 4.6 Pretend to read a book by telling a story from the pictures 9 4.6 Speak clearly enough to be understood by others 8 4.1 Recognize own name in writing 8 4.1 Recognize lower case letters 8 4.1 Articulate intents, emotions, and desires 6 3.0 Identify common objects 6 3.0 Understand that print contains a message 5 2.5 Use descriptive language 5 2.5 Recognize Iogographic print 4 2.0 Use appropriate expressive vocabulary 4 2.0 Retell story details 4 2.0 135 Table 17 (cont’d). ' Skill f % of re_spgnses Recognize upper case letters 3 1.5 Write some letters of the alphabet 3 1.5 Pretend to read a book 2 1.0 Distinguish between real and make-believe 2 1.0 Read others’ facial/body expressions 2 1.0 Recognize rhyming words 2 1.0 Respond to oral language with relevant comments and questions 2 1.0 Follow print on the page, moving eyes in the correct direction 2 1.0 Understand the difference between pictures and print 1 0.5 Use inventedltemmragy sgIIing 1 0.5 The majority of teachers did not agree on the top five emerging literacy skills children should have upon kindergarten entry. The highest percentage of teachers indicated fllat follow simple directions (10.2%) was the most important emerging literacy skill children should have upon kindergarten entry. This was followed by listen attentively to stories (9.1%). The emerging literacy skills indicated by the teachers were mostly oral language and concept of print skills. 136 Question Nine Are kindergarten teacher expectations of emerging literacy skills to be developed during the kindergarten year congruent with the State of Michigan Framework of Skills in the kindergarten year? The percentage of teacher agreement with the Michigan Curriculum Framework: Kindergarten Expectations (These standards are based on the Kindergarten English Language Arts Grade Level Content Expectations found in the Michigan Curricular Framework, Michigan Department of Education, 2004) is presented in Table 18 in descending order. 137 Table 18 Teacher Agreement With the Michigan Curriculum Framework: mm (N= 42) Framework Mflm Write some words Understand the difference between letters and words Associate letter names with their sounds Understand that words are separated by spaces Identify beginning sounds Write last name Develop a sight vocabulary Use invented/temporary spelling Understand where to begin reading Follow the print on a page moving eyes in the right direction Match spoken words with written ones Understand that letters are formed in a specific way and are unchanging Write a sentence Retell story details Recognize lower case letters Retell a familiar story Identify end sounds Develop a concept of punctuation Understand that print contains a message Recognize upper case letters Choose literary experiences when given a choice Respond to oral language with relevant comments and questions Use descriptive language Articulate intents, emotions, and desires Use appropriate expressive language Engage in conversation with peers with multiple exchanges Listen attentively to stories Speak clearly enough to be understood by others Engage in conversation with adults with multiple exchanges Follow simple directions 138 % of teacher ggreement 92.9 92.9 90.5 90.5 88.1 85.7 85.7 83.3 83.3 81.0 78.6 78.6 73.8 73.8 73.8 69.0 69.0 66.7 64.3 64.3 50.0 45.2 40.5 35.7 28.6 28.6 19.0 14.3 7.1 7.1 Teachers agree that the majority of emerging literacy skills in the Michigan Curriculum Framework: Kindergarten Expectations should be developed in kindergarten. However, there are nine skills that less than 50 percent of teachers agreed should be developed in kindergarten. As was seen earlier, teachers indicated that they believe these skills should be developed prior to kindergarten entry. Additional Findiggs A majority of kindergarten teachers (78.6%) in this sample believed that boys and girls have equal emerging literacy skills. Of those who did not think this was true, about 24 percent thought that girls’ emerging literacy skill development surpassed boys’ and about 2 percent of teachers believed boys’ literacy skill development surpassed girls’. Another finding of this study is that the majority of kindergarten teachers (90.5%) have had no contact with childcare programs, developmental kindergarten programs (73.8%), or private preschool programs (83.3%). Many have, however, been in contact with Head Start or MSRP programs (52.4%). In a related finding, the majority of kindergarten teachers reported that they never asked for student portfolios and, as expected, reported that they did not receive them from the majority of programs. According to these kindergarten teachers, 45 percent of Head Start or MSRP programs sent portfolios, about 17 percent of developmental kindergarten programs sent portfolios, about 10 percent of private preschools sent portfolios, and no childcare program sent a portfolio to teachers whether they asked or not. 139 Summag In this section, the results were presented according to each question. The research questions in this study examined the beliefs of kindergarten teachers in regard to when they thought certain emerging literacy skills should be developed in young children. As a result of the analysis, it appears that kindergarten teachers agree on the timing of development as regards many of the emerging literacy skills, however teachers may not have a complete understanding of the continuum of literacy development. The next chapter discusses the results of the study, as well as implications for classroom practice, implications for in-service and preservice teaching, and implications for research. 140 CHAPTER FIVE DISCUSSION This study investigated kindergarten teachers’ perceptions about when young children should best acquire emerging literacy skills. Teachers were asked to base their answers on when 75-80 percent of children are likely to develop each emerging literacy skill. Four underlying principles guided this study: a) emerging literacy skills are developed on a continuum over time (Neuman, Copple, 8r Bredekamp, 2000); b) explicit abilities necessary for reading and writing develop from direct encounters with both oral and written language (Sulzby & Teale, 1991); c) certain emerging literacy skills must be taught explicitly (Soderman, Gregory, & McCarty, 2005); and d) children’s literacy development is influenced by the literacy characteristics and expectations of those persons present and involved in the child’s immediate environment such as parents, childcare provider, preschool teacher, or kindergarten teacher who in turn are influenced by community, societal, and cultural factors (Bronfenbrenner, 1998). Discussion in this chapter centers on the three major sets of findings in this study: foundational skill recognition by kindergarten teachers; patterns of expectation of emerging literacy beliefs; and some additional factors investigated for teacher perspectives. Also considered in this chapter are the findings in relation to ecological perspectives. The chapter is concluded with implications for classroom practice, implications for in-service and preservice teaching, implications for future research, and the summary. 141 mm The process of becoming literate begins well before children enter formal schooling. Children do not become “real” readers until they have gained functional knowledge of the individual parts and pieces of literacy. These early skills, or foundational skills, include: language skills such as the ability to follow simple directions, repeat songs and fingerplays, follow movement activities, use appropriate expressive language; concept of print skills such as the ability to draw a recognizable picture, pretend to read a book, understand the difference between pictures and print, and understand that print carries a message; alphabetic skills such as the ability to identify letters of the alphabet and understand that letters are formed in a specific way and are unchanging; and phonological awareness skills, such as an ability to recognize rhyming words. The acquisition of these foundational skills falls in the emerging phase of the developmental continuum (Neuman, Copple, & Bredekamp, 2000; Soderman, Gregory, & McCarty, 2005) (see Figure 4). By indicating that 75-80 percent of children are likely to enter kindergarten with certain foundational oral language skills, the kindergarten teachers in this study showed an understanding that language is a crucial element in the development of emerging literacy (Hart & Risley, 1995). For example, over 90 percent of teachers indicated that children should be able to follow simple directions, about 83 percent said children should be able to repeat songs, fingerplays, and movement activities, and about 57 percent noted children should be able to use appropriate expressive language prior to kindergarten entry. 142 Teachers also indicated an understanding of the importance of drawing in the emerging literacy process as about 63 percent of teachers thought children should be able to draw a recognizable picture prior to kindergarten entry. However, kindergarten teachers in this sample appeared to have an incomplete understanding of which emerging literacy skills are foundational and the approximate timing of their development as explained below. Phonolggical Awareness About 83 percent of kindergarten teachers indicated they thought recognize rhyming words is a skill that should be developed after kindergarten entrance. Rhyming is a phonological skill that should be developed prior to kindergarten entry by way of nursery rhymes, rhyming games, poetry, and so on. Teachers may not understand that rhyming is a root of phonological awareness and must be developed as a basis for the development of more abstract phonological Wge and emerging reading skills (Anthony, Lonigan, 8 Dliscoll, 2003: MacLean, Bryant, & Bradley, 1987). The Committee on the Prevention of Reading Difficulties in Young Children (Snow, Burns, & Griffin, 1998) specifically recommends that home and preschool activities include songs, poems, and games that focus on rhyming. Comt of Print In another finding, about 60 percent of teachers noted that understanding the difference between pictures and print is a skill to be developed after kindergarten entrance, and 64 percent of teachers believed that Ieaming that print contains a message is a skill to be developed after kindergarten entrance. In contrast, 143 Christie et al. (1997) found that recognizing the difference between pictures and print, as well as the awareness that print has meaning are two of the initial discoveries that many children as young as three make about written language. Shared book reading from a very early age encourages verbal interaction that not only builds vocabulary, but also develops these types of concept of print skills. Alphabet Children’s alphabet knowledge upon school entry is one of the single strongest predictors of both short— and long-tenn literacy achievement (Adams, 1990; Stevenson & Newman, 1986). However, before children learn to identify letters of the alphabet, they must understand that letters are formed in a specific way and are unchanging. Yet, almost 79 percent of kindergarten teachers in this study indicated that this was a skill they believed should be developed after kindergarten entry. It is possible that teachers misunderstood the item, as more teachers reported that they should teach children that letters don’t change form than teach children to identify letters of the alphabet. Teachers in this study were asked to list the five most essential skills children should have developed prior to kindergarten entry. While it should be acknowledged that this was a difficult task, as there were numerous emerging skills to choose from, and so many of these skills are very important, it must also be noted that several of the foundational skills were missing from the compiled list. Absent from the top ten most commonly listed skills were foundational skills such as recognize rhyming words, understand that letters are formed in a specific and unchanging way, and understand the difference between print and pictures. 144 It should also be noted that the frequency of response for certain other foundational skills was very low. For example, only one percent of teachers said that pretend to read a book was one of the most important literacy skills children should have upon kindergarten entry. The low frequency of response for this and other skills may be due to the fact that there were so many skills to choose from and the obvious diversity in teacher thinking. Patterns of EMtions Analyses of the data uncovered several patterns of expectations of emerging literacy beliefs. In this study, teaching background, education background, and certain classroom characteristics were related to teacher perceptions of the development of emerging literacy skills. Teachigg Background One pattern noted pertains to differences in kindergarten teacher background in education and emerging literacy beliefs. For example, teachers with less than ten years of teaching experience had higher expectations for the development of certain concept of print skills as compared to teachers with 21 or more years in education. The data revealed that veteran teachers in this study thought that such skills as understand that words are separated by spaces and write a sentence should be developed in first grade, whereas the less experienced teachers said these skills should be developed after kindergarten entry. It may be that teachers’ views vary depending upon the particular cuniculum supplied by the school (Snow, et al., 1998) or simply their past experience. 145 The data in this study also uncovered the fact that kindergarten teachers with 1-25 years of preschool teaching experience reported holding higher expectations regarding the development of certain concept of print skills as compared to those kindergarten teachers with no preschool teaching experience. For instance, it was reported that kindergarten teachers with preschool teaching experience expected skills such as understand where to begin reading, write some letters, and follow the print on a page, moving the eyes in the correct direction to be developed prior to kindergarten entry. In contrast, kindergarten teachers with no preschool teaching experience expect these same skills to be developed after kindergarten entry. This may indicate that kindergarten teachers who have taught in preschool hold higher expectations for young children, as compared to kindergarten teachers who have never taught preschool, perhaps as a result of working with preschool children and observing the development of these skills at the preschool level. Education Background Another pattern found in the data pertained to kindergarten teachers with early childhood training. Specifically, those teachers with early childhood training (e.g. early childhood major, early childhood endorsement), were more likely to expect certain oral language skills to be developed later. Kindergarten teachers who had a background in early childhood were more likely to say that the oral language skills retell story details, articulate intents, emotions, and desires, and listen attentively to stories should be developed after kindergarten entry, whereas those teachers without an early childhood background were more likely to say 146 that these skills should be developed prior to kindergarten. The discrepancy may be due to teachers having a different understanding of the degree of difficulty of the skill. Some teachers may have perceived listen “attentively” to be a somewhat similar skill as “listen,” whereas teachers with an early childhood background may see listen “attentively” as being a more concentrated type of listening than “listen.” Notably, Snow, Burns, 8 Griffin (1998) specify that three- year-old children should be able to listen to stories while kindergartners should be able to listen attentively to books. A somewhat similar pattern was found regarding kindergarten teachers who participate in professional activities and those who do not For example, those teachers who were members of the National Association for the Education of Young Children (NAEYC), attended NAEYC or other conferences, and subscribed to professional journals indicated they were more likely to believe children should develop the ability to follow print on a page, move eyes in the correct direction, identify middle sounds, and recognize lower case letters later than those teachers who were not members or subscribers. For example, those teachers who attended professional conferences were more likely to say that recognize lower case letters is a skill that should be developed after kindergarten enhance as compared to those who did not attend conferences, who indicated this skill should be developed prior to kindergarten entry. It should be noted that the majority of teachers in this study (71.4%) attend conferences, and a large majority of those who attend (90.4%) say that professional conferences are an influential or highly influential source of their expectations for children’s emerging 147 literacy. It appears from this study that teachers who attend conferences and/or belong to professional organizations have different understandings of how children’s emerging literacy skills should be appropriately supported than those who do not. Classroom Characteristics A few classroom characteristics were noted as being associated with certain patterns of teacher perceptions. For instance, teachers who taught in a half-day program had higher expectations for the development of certain concept of print skills as compared to those teachers who taught in all day, every day programs. Teachers in the all day, every day programs were more likely to expect such skills as distinguish between real and make-believe and write first name to be developed after kindergarten entry, whereas the teachers in the half-day programs were more likely to say these skills should be developed prior to kindergarten entry. it is important to keep in mind that the all day, every day programs in this study were mainly from urban areas, so it could be that, rather than the length of day, that explains the discrepancy in perceptions. Another classroom characteristic of some significance in this study is class size. Class size has recently been at the forefront of the list of possible solutions to closing the literacy-Ieaming gap between groups of diverse children. In this study, the number of students in the classroom affected teachers” perspectives on the development of certain oral language and concept of print skills. Kindergarten teachers with smaller class sizes (15-20 students) were more likely to say that certain oral language and concept of print skills should be developed 148 sooner than teachers with larger class sizes (21-27 students). A steady decline in the average number of students in elementary school classrooms is seen nationally; however, the average number of students in poor urban classrooms is still above those of any other type (NCES, 2002). According to Snow et al. (1998), though, the effects of reduced class size are mixed. Several studies have found that a reduction in class size did not necessarily result in increased sustained reading achievement effects. Perhaps the differences found here are not necessarily strictly from a class size variation. There may be other, unidentified factors related to teachers with different class sizes that influence teacher perspectives. This study found differences in teacher perceptions of the development of emerging literacy skills as a function of children’s prior program experience. For instance, kindergarten teachers who thought that none of their students had prior private preschool experience were more likely to say that children should develop the emerging literacy skills recognize Iogographic print and identify common objects prior to kindergarten, whereas those kindergarten teachers who said they had at least 25 percent or more students who attended a private preschool indicated these skills should be developed after kindergarten entrance. Teachers having children with no private prescth experience indicated that the skill identify middle sounds should be developed after kindergarten entry, as opposed to those teachers with 1-75 percent of students with private preschool experience who said that was a skills that should be developed during early first grade. Teachers may have varied expectations for students with different program 149 experiences because they are unaware of the experiences that children have during those programs. Kindergarten teachers with little or no communication with children’s preschool programs probably do not know what curricula are offered. In this study, 83 percent of kindergarten teachers indicated they had no contact with any private preschools. Additional Factors Investlg' ated for Teacher Perspeity' es Additional factors investigated in this study proving to have some agreement among kindergarten teachers are: sources of teacher influence on expectations of children’s literacy readiness, congruency of kindergarten teacher beliefs of children’s literacy readiness with others, congruency of kindergarten teacher beliefs with their school district standards and demands, and the Michigan Curriculum Framework of kindergarten expectations. Influences on Teacher's tions Teachers reported that there are many fairly influential sources for kindergarten teachers’ expectations of children’s literacy readiness. However, only one source, past teaching experience, was indicated by a majority of teachers as a very influential source. Research has indicated that as teachers become more experienced, they tend to use, among oflrer things, what has worked in the past, accordingly building a belief system for making classroom decisions (Stephens 8 Clyde, 1995). Attendance at professional conferences was the second most commonly reported influence on teachers’ expectations of children’s literacy skills. About 71 percent of kindergarten teachers in this study attended conferences. Therefore, 150 this may be an important finding with implications for delivering educative information to teachers. In a similar vein, teachers indicated that in-service training was a fairly influential source for their expectations, as well. Again, if teachers are using these modes of information delivery as influential sources, more attention should be paid to the quality and quantity of information that is delivered to teachers. These are two obviously powerful methods of conveying what teachers need for professional development around literacy expectations. Teachers also indicated that the (Michigan) state curriculum framework, as a source of expectations of children’s literacy development, was fairly influential. In addition, teachers identified Federal legislation as a somewhat influential source for kindergarten teachers’ expectations of children’s literacy readiness. This may not be the case in the future as the impact of the NCLB Act is felt more deem)!- Co_rlgrueppy of Beliefs Rpggrdipg Children’s LEM Capabilities This study also looked at congruency of teacher beliefs regarding children’s literacy capabilities as compamd to others’ beliefs. The data found that these particular kindergarten teachers supposed their beliefs to be mostly congruent with other kindergarten teachers’ beliefs about children’s literacy capabilities. The data indicated that teachers thought their beliefs were somewhat congruent with first grade teachers’ beliefs, standards set by the school district, the state curriculum framework, parents’ beliefs, standards set by Federal legislation, and preschool teachers’ beliefs about children’s literacy capabilities. 151 Notably, kindergarten teachers said their beliefs about children’s literacy capabilities were somewhat congruent with parents’ expectations. Much current literature has shown that there exist discontinuities of literacy practices and expectations among different social and cultural groups, as well as between home and school (e.g., Au 8 Manson, 1981; Delgado-Gaitan 8 Trueba, 1997; Heath, 1983; Paratore, Melzi, 8 Krol-Sinclair, 1999; Philips, 1983; Schmidt, 1998; Volk; 1997; Xu, 1999). Teachers are not necessarily knowledgeable about differences in family literacy practices, often presuming the practices in middle- class homes to be the norm. Teachers in this study indicated that their beliefs are in somewhat of an agreement with parents, perhaps acknowledging some discontinuity does exist between teachers and parents. Mm of Beliefs with Local School District About 63 percent of teachers indicated that their beliefs about children’s literacy capabilities were congruent to highly congruent with the standards set by the school district. Eighty percent or more of teachers indicated that their beliefs regarding 21 out of the 28 emerging literacy skills they are responsible for teaching were consistent with district demand. Note that these two questions ask different things of teachers—in one case about congruence of their beliefs as a whole and in the other case congruence of beliefs on individual skills. We can generalize across the two that for many teachers there is continuity on many or all items, but for some teachers, and some items, discontinuity exists. 152 Michigan Curriculum Framework of Mflom Kindergarten teacher beliefs were compared regarding when emerging literacy skills should be developed against the Michigan Curriculum Framework: Kindergarten Expectations (Michigan Department of Education, 2004). It is important to note that these expectations had come out shortly before this survey was administered. The kindergarten teachers in this sample agreed with the state expectations on 21 out of the 30 skills—that they should be developed after kindergarten entrance. The majority of these skills are concept of print skills. The eight skills for which the teachers did not agree with the state expectations are mostly oral language skills with teachers expecting these skills to be developed before kindergarten entry. However, it is important for teachers to understand that they must continue to support oral language skill development. Some children come to kindergarten well behind in oral language skills (Hart 8 Risley, 1995). Where kindergarten teachers in this study indicated the majority of skills they were responsible for teaching are concept of print skills, may must also continue to support oral language skill development. W The person—process-context design of this study guided an investigation of contextual and personal characteristics that might possibly influence kindergarten teacher beliefs about emerging literacy (Bronfenbrenner, 1998). The kindergarten teachers in this study reported that aspects of the microsystem such as past experience, education background, and teaching background; aspects of the mesosystem such as in-service training and certain classroom 153 characteristics; aspects of the exosystem, such as professional conferences and state standards; and aspects of the macrosystem such as federal legislation were influential in forming their emerging literacy beliefs (see Figure 2). From an ecological viewpoint, this study substantiated that different ecological niches can be influential in teacher perspectives regarding emerging literacy skills. Implications for Classroom Practice This section includes several suggestions for classroom and teacher support to enhance young children’s literacy Ieaming. 1. While this study showed that teachers in general did not ask for, nor receive, portfolios, they are a method of authentic assessment that would give kindergarten teachers insight as to where children’s literacy development falls on the continuum even before school starts in the fall. By communicating with preschool, and even childcare programs that feed into the school district, the goal of expecting and receiving portfolios for children could be ananged. Kindergarten teachers would then be able to further plan for the developmental levels of the incoming students. However, teachers must also be trained in creating and using portfolios as a means of Ieaming about children and their developmental levels. 2. Communication across kindergarten classrooms, and also with first grade classrooms, concerning constancy of expectations for students would aid in continuing teacher perspectives along the same path. Kindergarten teachers must have time to meet, not only among themselves but with preschool and first grade teachers as well, and not only as a group in a 154 school, but also as a group within the district. Of course, meetings should be focused on research-based expectations of literacy development. Implications for lrl-JService and Preservice Education As a result of this study, it can be noted that kindergarten teachers might have an incomplete understanding of the development of young children’s literacy. A large part of teacher education comes from in-service and preservice Ieaming. Therefore, those accountable for teacher training should be proactive in ensuring emerging literacy awareness. This section includes several suggestions for in- service and preservice teacher support to enhance young children’s literacy learning. 1. It is important that teachers be well versed regarding the development of literacy skills. As children with very divergent skills characterize today’s kindergarten classes, teachers must be prepared to support children regardless of their literacy skill development level. It is crucial that teachers understand where children are developmentally, and they must realize it is their responsibility to support the development of those essential skills that must be present in order for children to progress along a somewhat sequential literacy continuum. In—service training regarding the developmental progression of emerging literacy skill development would be beneficial for teachers to review their understanding of this pattern and would also to help avoid 155 inappropriate teaching practices. Teachers also need the support of administration for continuous support of appropriate practices. . The majority of teachers in this study indicated that they attend professional conferences and that that these conferences are influential sources on their expectations. Professional early education organizations such as the NAEYC and IRA have an important responsibility to disseminate information about the developmental continuum of reading and writing. These organizations can also play an important role in developing teachers’ competence in a range of strategies needed to support and assess children’s emerging literacy (IRA 8 NAEYC, 2000). . Educators responsible for preservice training must ensure that prospective teachers understand developmental progressions of children’s literacy, beginning particularly with language development. Knowledge of the foundational emerging literacy skills that are necessary for the development of more abstract skills is essential for all teachers. Knowing ways in which children might vary in the order of literacy skills development is also important. By ensuring that novice teachers start out with strong preservice preparation in this area, there is a greater likelihood teachers will use appropriate practices and children will have appropriate experiences. 156 4. Portfolio use should be introduced by both in-service and preservice educators. Practicing and preservice teachers ought to be made aware of the many benefits of a portfolio program. Portfolios can be used as an on-going assessment of the Ieaming process, as a means of collaboration and communication between teachers, students, and parents, and as a showcase of a child’s individual accomplishments, Ieaming progress, and skills (Soderman, Gregory, 8 McCarty, 2005). Portfolios should be used and passed along across grade levels. For this to happen there must be communication among preschools, kindergarten, and first grades at the classroom, building, and district level. Implications for Future Research This section includes several suggestions for future research related to teacher perceptions of children’s emerging literacy skills. 1. The sample for this study did not have an equal number of teachers from rural, urban, and suburban areas. Because of the limits of representation, some significant teacher perspectives may not have been discovered. This study should be replicated using a larger and more diverse sample. 2. The sample for this study was made up of kindergarten teachers who chose to fill out the survey and mail it back. Self-selection for this study may have limited responses, and therefore a replication of this study using a compelled sample or interview protocol may result in differing data. 157 3. The discussion of the results of this study is based on teacher self-report. A study using teacher observation and interview would illuminate whether and/or how emerging literacy perspectives of kindergarten teachers are put into pracfice. 4. A study comparing expectations of preschool teachers, kindergarten teachers, and first grade teachers who serve the same children over time would illuminate similarities and differences in teacher perceptions across different age groups served, and provide some insight into some issues children may face in moving through these different settings. 5. In this sample, a majority of teachers said that their beliefs concerning when individual emerging literacy skills should be developed were congruent with their districts demands. Future study might look at whether teachers’ beliefs really are congruent with district demand. It is not known whether the teachers’ view of district demand is really congnrent with their school district. It is only known that teachers think it is. This type of study would be particularly helpful for district in-services, as well as lesson continuity. 6. Because parents and childcare providers are also important influences on children's literacy development (Snow, et al., 1998), discovering the perspectives of parents and childcare providers concerning emerging literacy skill development would aid in identifying expectation similarities and differences. 158 7. Wl‘lh the passing of the No Child Lelt Behind Act of 2001(NCLB), states, and subsequently school districts, have had to start changing policies and practices to meet NCLB guidelines. Future research could investigate changes in teacher perspectives regarding emerging literacy skills over the course of time as more states and school districts come into compliance with Federal mandates. film This investigation determined that the responding kindergarten teachers had some but not a complete understanding of the developmental path of emerging literacy skills. There seemed to be a lack of awareness as to when certain foundational emerging literacy skills should be developed and the impact of those skills on the development of other, more abstract literacy skills. This presents implications for both experienced teachers and preservice teachers in assuring their understanding of when and how emerging literacy skills develop in young children. Findings from the present study indicate that kindergarten teachers agreed on when many of young children’s emerging literacy skills should be developed. The teachers in this study agreed that most of the skills that they were responsible for teaching are concept of print skills, as well as some alphabetic principle skills, phonological awareness skills, phonemic awareness skills, and a few developmental communication skills. The majority of skills that they 159 expected 75-80 percent of children to develop prior to kindergarten entry were oral language skills. While the teachers in this study agreed on many items of emerging literacy development, there was also noted disagreement among specific subgroups of teachers regarding the development of certain emerging literacy skills. Particularly, differences among teacher perspectives relating to several personal characteristics, as well as contextual characteristics, emerged. These include: teaching background; education background; and certain classroom characteristics including program type, class size, and children’s prior program experience. Finally, several factors surfaced that had teacher agreement including: sources of teacher influence on expectations of children’s literacy readiness, congruency of kindergarten teacher beliefs of children’s literacy readiness with others, congruency of kindergarten teacher beliefs with their school district standards and demands, and the Michigan Curriculum Framework of kindergarten expectations. 160 APPENDICES 161 APPENDIX A Grade Level Content Expectations: Kindergarten English Language Arts 162 Grade Level Content Expectations: Kindergarten English Language Arts SECTION OF SKILL DEVELOPMENT EXPECTATION EXPECTATION READING Students will recognize that words are “'0"! ROCOQNIfiON composed of sounds blended together, and can do tasks that demonstrate phonemic awareness. Students understand the alphabetic principle-that the sounds in words are expressed by the letters of the alphabet. Students will use grapho-phonemic (letter- sound) cues to recognize a few one-syllable words and have begun to associate letters and sounds, particularly initial and final consonants. Students will recognize frequently encountered words in print automatically. Students should be beginning to learn the 220 Dolch basic sight vocabulary. Students will know the meaning of words encountered frequently in kindergarten in oral language contexts. Student will follow written text while pointing to matching words: notice when simple sentences fail to make sense; make predictions based on illustrations or portions of stories; and use picture clues, patterns of language, or initial letters/sounds to narrow possibilities in predictingwords. Students will apply the following aspects of fluency: retelling familiar text and recognizing_grade level specific words. Students will determine the meaning of words and phrases (e.g., objects, actions, concepts, and content and English language arts vocabulary) in context using strategies and resources (picture clues, prediction, and other people. 163 Narrative Text Students will identify and describe a variety of genre (e.g., nursery rhymes, poetry, songs»). Students will discuss simple story elements (e.g., setting, character, events) in narrative text. Students will identify how authors use pictures, illustrations to support the understanding of settings and characters. Informational Text Students will identify and describe a variety of informational genre (e.g., environmental text, concept books, picture books). Students will discuss information text patterns (e.g., sequential, descriptive). Students will explain how authors use picture, drawings, and icons to enhance the understanding of supporting and key ideas present in descriptive (i.e. definitions, enumeration) and sequential (directions, steps, procedures) organizational patterns. Comprehension Students will connect personal knowledge and experience to ideas in texts. Students will retell in sequence from books and stories they have encountered often, from simple oral and/or written narrative and informational text (i.e., no more than three events in the sequence of reading), using their own words or phrasing. Students will begin to make comparisons and connections across oral and written text and personal experience. Students will acquire and use significant knowledge from what has been read and experienced in kindergarten, including grade- Ievel appropriate science and social studies knowledge, and uses this knowledge to make connections to new texts. Metacognilion Students will self-rnonitor comprehension when reading grade-level texts (i.e., be aware if one is understanding what is being read) and show some use of simple strategies to increase comprehension (e.g., make credible predictions about the content of a reading based on a preview of the reading, including the cover of a book and its pictures). 164 Students will plan, monitor, regulate and evaluate skills, strategies and processes to construct and convey meaning (e.g., notice when sentences fail to make sense), use story grammar to identify author’s perspective and begin to sort and order information with extensive teacher guidance. Critical Standards Students will recognize how to assess personal works and the work of others with teacher supervision. Reading Attitude Students are enthusiastic about reading and learning how to read. Students choose books, book activities, word play and writing on their won, that is, during free time in school and at home. WRITING Writing Genres Students will write brief nanative pieces (e.g., fairy tales, nursery rhymes or songs) using pictures and/or illustrations, and words or work-like clusters as support; brief informational pieces (e.g., a page for a class writing assignment) using text, pictures, drawings, and/or illustrations; and contribute to a class research project by restating the class research question in their own words, selecting appropriate teacher-supplied texts for support, organizing gathered information in a correct sequence, using steps in the writing process to produce a final discourse, and presenting the finished research project. Process Students will consider audience needs as they plan their writing, including brainstorming to help them generate and structure ideas for writing narrative as well as informational text, with teacher assistance as needed. Students will use phonetic spelling to represent narrative and informational text when drafting, incorporating pictures, drawings and illustrations. Students will revise writing by reading their own aloud and requesting additions or clarifications that support meaninL 165 Both individually and in groups, students will attempt to edit their writing by reading aloud their writing/picture and use a grade 1 list. Personal Style Students will display originality in oral, written and visual messages (e.g., in nanative text; natural language, expressed sentiment, original ideas; in informational text: listing, naming, describing). Spelling Students will spell frequently encountered one-syllable words correctly. For less frequently encountered words, students will rely more on structural clues (i.e., letter/sound) than environmental sources such as words walls or words lists. For such lower frequency words, students will strategically focus on beginning and some of the simpler ending sounds more than on sounds in the middle. Handwriting Students will form upper and lower case letters, use known or copied words in writing, leave space between words and word-like clusters of letters, write from left to right and top to bottom, and experiment with representing ideas and information using written symbols. Writing Attitude Students are enthused about writing and Ieaming how to write. SPEAKING Conventions Students will use language to communicate with a variety of audiences and for different purposes (e.g., courtesies, daily conversations, playground/classroom interactions). Students will speak clearly and audibly in complete coherent sentences, use sound effects, and use illustrations when presenting spoken informational and narrative text. Students make presentations or reports in standard American English. 166 Students will be aware that language differs from playground and classroom as a function of linguistic and cultural group membership. They can provide examples of language differences on the playground and in the classroom. Spoken Discourse Students will tell/retell brief familiar experiences or interests, covering the main story grammar points emphasizing elaborated setting and character information while speaking clearly and audibly in complete sentences. Students will respond to multiple text types by discussing, drawing, and/or writing in order to reflect, make meaning and make connections. Students will plan and deliver presentations or reports using informational organizational pattern (e.g., descriptive) and appropriate text features (e.g., pictures, illustrations), providing several facts and details to make their point while speaking clearly and audibly in complete sentences. LISTENINGNIEWING Conventions (Interpersonal Processes) Students will understand and follow one- and two-step directions. Students will ask appropriate questions during a presentation or report. Students will listen to each other and interact appropriately. Students will use effective listening and viewing behaviors. Students will begin to evaluate messages they experience, Ieaming to differentiate between sender and receiver. Response Students will listen to or view and discuss a variety of genres. Students will listen to, view and respond thoughtfully to both classic and contemporary texts recognized forgualig and literary merit. 167 APPENDIX B Emergent Literacy Skills Survey 168 EMERGING LITERACY SKILLS SURVEY Thankyoulorparticipatinginthissurvey. Pleasebeascandidaspossible. Allofyouransverswillbe anonyrmusandconfidenlial. Yourhoncstyandthoroughnccsm’llensureamorevalidstudy. Demographic Inforrnatlon: Please couplets the following: 1. Your age 2. Your ethnicity/race _Alrican-American _American Indian _Asian—Pacilic _Hispanic _While _Other 3. Total number of years in education 4. Number of years teaching kindergarten 10. 11. 12. 13. 14. 15. 16. 17. 18. Numberofyearsteachingpeschoolchildren ThemajorareaofconcentrationofyourBA/BS Highestdegreeeamed DoyouhaveanEa'lyChildhoodendorsenent? Yes No AreyouamemberofflleNafimdAssociafionbrflleEducafiononwrgCIildren? Yes_No Doyousubscribetooneorrnoreprofcssiondeducationjoumals? Yes No Doyouafiendprofessionaleducafionconlerenccs?(0heckallfllatmply) Yes No ACEI IRA MiAEYC NAEYC MRA Other Howmalyhoursofpmfcssionaldevelopmthaveyouhadhthelastyear? Locationofyourcunentschool Rural Suburban Urban Whattypeotprogramdoyouhavethisyear? 1I2day allday,everyday alday,everyotherday Howrnaryclridmnareirryourclassroornlhisyea?(onave.,ifyouhavemoretharoneclass)_ I-Iovrmanychildreninyourclassroomthisyearale 5-years-old? 6-ycas—old? 7- years-old? mmmmddfldrenmakhgmmdacsrmflisyear _%AlricalAmerican_%Asian %Hispanic %WhitelCaucasian %Olher AppmxinatehMpemntagedchfldmnmceivesfieeamdweduchhmdassrounmis year? 169 19. Pleaselilloutthe Head Start or Private No preschool Do not expensnce kirmrgarten Michigan preschool experience know School Readiness Program Estimated it of children in your classroom this year that have had a program experience prior to kindergarten entry Have you had contact with the _Yes _Yes _Yes _Yes program that your shldenls may have experienced? _No _Nc _Nc _Nc Have you asked for any portfolios _Yes _Yes _Yes _Yes Item the following programs? _No _Nc _No _Nc Have you received my portfolios from _Yes _Yes _Yes _Yes the following programs? _Nc _Nc _No _No 20. Howinfluentielhaveeachofthefollowingbeenininfonningyouofexpectationsofchildren‘sliteracy madiness? Not at all Influential Assessrnentusedbythedistrict Expectations of first grade teachers Federal legislation How I was taught as a child In-service training Local scth board Media sources (e.g., TV, radio, newspapers) Other kindergarten teachers Past teaching experience Professional conferences Professional journals School district cuniculum State Curriculum Framework 1 _L—L—fi—L—L—b‘d—L—Lu—b—L—L N NNNNNNNNNNNNN 170 SomcMrat Influential 3 wwwwwwwwwwwww & hh##h####hh## Very influential 5 mmmmmmmmmmmmm 21. Tomadegmeaeymrbeielsdruudfldmn’sibracycmdrificscugnmwimeachdfle blowing? Notctchongruent Somcwlracongrucnt I-Iighlycongrucnt 1 2 3 4 5 Preschoolteachers’expectations 1 2 3 4 5 Olherkindergaltenteachers’expedations 1 2 3 4 5 Firstgradeteachers’expectations 1 2 3 4 5 Paents’expectations 1 2 3 4 5 Stardardssetbymyschooldistrict 1 2 3 4 5 StateCuniculumFramemrk 1 2 3 4 5 StmdadssetbyFederallegislation 1 2 3 4 5 22. Pleasedlcckvnidloneofthefolovringstatementsyoubelievetobehue: _Borsafleibhaveemflmmmskilmm l. _GIIIS'MWWSWWWNW _Boys’ emerging literacy skill development surpasses gill 23. Pbaseistbebwflefimnnsthwhrfienergkglitemcyddflsdfldmnsluldhavelpon kindergartenentry(with#1beinglhemostirnportantek:. Youmaylefertoqucsticn24,ifyouwish): 1. 171 24. FolowingareemergingHeracyskillsmadevebphdfldmnbetweenfleagesof3ard6yeasdage. Sarremaybeconsidered‘readiness’skillsflratslnuldbepfimafilydevebpedbythetimeachildreaches kindergarten. Odersnaybecasidemdsfllbmadevebpmrwflyaflerflndergartenenhacemmealy firstglade. Pleasedesignaetheperiodinwhich,basedonyourexperbnce,75%toBO%ofchiflrenwillikelyhave theseskills, by placing a check mark in thatcolumn only. For example: Emerging Skill should be Skill should be primarily Skill should be Is this thcracy Skills primarily dcvclopcd developed Mar primarily comm with priorto kindergatcn kindergarten crltr'lllcc dcvcloped during district demand cntrance first Jradc Yeslllo 1. Write a story x No Ernerg'ngljbracySkills Skillshould Skillshouldbe Skilshould Isllris be primarly primarily be primaily consistent developed developed after developed with rislrict priorto kinderga'ten drlring first demald kindergaten entrmce gade entralce Yes/No 1. Recogrize lower case letters 2. Engageinconversalionwithadullswith multiple exchanges 3. Draw recognizable picture (sell, family, objects. We) 4. Exhibit reading-like behavior (e.g., turn correctly, movingfiom front to back) 5. Distinguish betvveen real and make-believe 6. Pretendtoreadabookbytellingastory fromthepicturesonlhecoverorinlhebook 7. Repeat songs, fingerplays, and movement 8. Follow simple directions 9. Understand that print contains a message 10. Use descriptive Iarrguage (e.g., I want to plpy with the smdl, red truck) 11. Followlheprintonapage, movingeyes ill the correct direction 12. Respond to oral language with relevant comments and questions 13. Retell story details 14. Use irrventedllemporary spelling 15. Understand where to mmadipg 16. Use appropriate expressive vocabulary 17. Understand that words are sepaated by spaces 18. Recognize upper case letters 19. Develgp a concept of punctuation 172 Skill should be primarily developed prior to kindergart en entrance Skill should be primarily developed alter kindergaten entrmce Islhis withl'l'l demand Yes/No 20 Writelirslna'ne 21: Recognize rhyming words 22 Listenattenlivelytostories 23. Identify end sounds 24. Articulate intents, emotions, and desires 25. Identify letters of the alphdret 26. Understand lhatlettersareformed in a specific wayand are unchangiflg 27. Choose literacy experiences when gimn a choice 28. Identify ginnigg sounds 29. Write a sentence 30. Recognize Iogographic print (e.g., McDonalds, Kmart) 31. Associate Ietternarneswillrtlehsounds 32. Recognize common nouns and verbs typical of daily life 33. Understandlhedilferencebelween picturesandprint 34. Speakclearlyenoughtobeunderstood byothers 35. Retell a familiar story 36. Understandlhedifferencebetweenletters andwords 37. Matdrspokenwordswilhwrittenones 38. Write last name 39. Read others’ facial and bodyexpressions 40. Formulate simple questions 41. Write some letters of the dphabet 42. Engage in conversation with peers with multiple exchgges 43. Break words into sylldlles 44. Develop a sight vocabulary 45. Writesomewords 46. Use right-to-lelt, top-to-boltom, and front- to-back orientation in book ream 47. Identify common objects (e.g., vehicles) 48. Recognize own name in writng 49. Identify middle sounds 50. ldenlifylilleofabook 173 APPENDIX C Letter of Teacher Consent 174 KINDERGARTEN TEACHER PERCEPTIONS REGARDING THE DEVELOPMENT (1: YOUNG CHILDREN'S EMERGENT LITERACY SKILLS DeaKindergarten Teacher, lnadechdedvdybrheamecsmllimmcydewbmefidmiumnmbbeefiddbundesmnd drefiteracybfiefsofmeadultsindrargeofchildren’semergingliteracy. Kindergarbnteachersplayar ’lnporlantloleinthedevelopmentofchildren'sliteracy. Therefore, itiscrucialthattlreirbeliefsbe recognizedanddiscussed. AsakindergartenteacherinlngharnCounty,youhavebeenselectedto pafidpabinhbshfly,flichmmsflgamsdepempfimmflmmabmexlersmgadingmndey memmbrbmhimspedflcemmmskibbm3b6yeasdage. Theatachedsuveytdtesqrplox'lnaerISmirutesbflout AseIf-addrecsedstanpedemlelopeis providedasameansofmhrrnirgthesunreybtherescachers. Paticipalionisvoluntary. Therewilheno pendtyumnseqracesiyoudnosendbaswuddfleqnflasuflyoudmsendbpafidpae atall. lheirbnnaiongaheredfiornflleslmyswibehededwifllshictcmfidence. Youwilnotbeidentilidrle inmyleportofresealchfindings. Yourprivacywilbeprotecledtolhemaxirnunextentdlowablebylaw. Onlytheescercherswillhaveaccecstolhecollecteddata. lfyouhaveanyquestionsorcormrns regarding this study, please contact the investigators at Dr. Anne Soderman, e-ma'l: soderman@msu.edu, phone: (517) 432-0692, Peggy Thelen, e-ma'l: thelenpe@msu.edu, phone: (989) 463-6041 or Mchelle Nicholson, atthe IISD, e-ma'l: mnicho|s@ingmamisd.org, phone: (517) 244-1384. fiwuhammquamnsmgadmmnghsasashflypafidpataaemedamy tirnewilh anyaspectofthis study, you maycontact-anonymously, ifyouwish-PeterVasilenko, Ph.D., Cha’r of the University Committee on Research Involving Human Subjects (UCRIHS) by plrorle: (517) 355-2180, fax (517) 432-4503, e-mai: ucrihs@msu.edu, or regula ma'l: 202 Olds hdl, East Larsing, Ml 48824. Asagcshrmdqrpreddionbryourcmsmmflflommeamdndsuwcy,youwlflhwcdle Wflymparddpateinadnwlngforoneofburfiommflcelflficatesfiunabcdbookstore. Yummmwmmwmmmmmmwam Orrcereceived,mehveshgabrwiflseparatehrecmsemletterfiunflesuwey. Allconsentletterswillbe putinasecurecontainer. Fwnaneswiflbedrawnhomdroseparhcbatirgbachersbreceiveagifi certificab. hyouwishbpafidpabhfledmwing,pbasemdbateyournam,sdm,adsdbddishbt belowardretunthislebrwithyourswvey. Ifyouchooserxrtbpaticbdeillhedrawilgs’cmlylehm onlythesurvey. HcascrshrmdlesurvcytomcbyAprll .2004. Yourpultcipdlonisgcdlywprccldcd. Flntirrgswlbcuccdtodcccrnwhdirqrcctexperlcnce “Mnghmcmlkescybdidsawcflammmmmbr prcschoolandklndcrwtcntccchcrs. Mt PeggyTlmn Ph.D.Cardidw If I' Stall' 'I Name SI I D‘I'I 175 APPENDIX D Principal Letter 176 InmefioflbbetbrwdestaflibmcypempfiomdflndegabnbademhlmhanCwfiy,Peggy Thelen, a graduate studentat Michigan State University, in cooperation with the Ingham Intermediate School District (IISD) is facilitating a literacy study in Inghan County. This study will ascertain those emerging literacy skills kindergarten teachers thinkshould beprirnalily developed priortokindergarten entry, during kindergarten, and during early first grade. EverykindergartenteacherirlnghanCouMywillleceiveasurveybymail. Themyswillbeiehrrnedb theinvesligatorbymail. lhellSDwilmceivedatagameredfiornflresuweys,wlidiflrenmaybeusedb designpersonneldevelopment Tliekibrnrafiongaleiedfiundiesllveysrwlbeheaeduiihshictcmfideme. Teacherswillnotbe identifiableinanyreportofresealchfindings.lheirprivacywrllbeprotectedtothemaximumexterrt I dowablebylaw. Onlytheresearclierswillhaveaccesstothecollecteddata. Ifycuhaveanyquestionsor F‘ concemsregardingthisstudy,pleasecontacttheinvestigatorsat Dr.AnneSodennan,e—mail: ' soderman@msu.edu, phone: (517) 432-0692, Peggy Thelen, e-ma'l: thelenpe@msu.edu, phone: (989) 463- 6041 or Michelle Nicholson, attire lnghan ISD, e-mail: mnichols@inqhamisd.org, phone: (517) 244-1384. Inadebmdrehbshdymmsuwecsblmeaeasfinguhdpdsbsmmfiwefiubbhaveasmmy kindergartenteachersaspossiblefilloutandiehrmlhesurvey. Youreflortstosupportlhisproject,by geiflymnwmteaclembfilmnflesuweymdmmmflemhafinelymammbuflbegmdly arpreciated. Teaclielsumiemmdesuveyswilhavealoppommnybwinagificerfificatebabcal bookstore. TherehmdateisApii .2004. lfyou hawmyquesfiusmcacens,uaedbsdisfiedamyfinbwimmyaspeddflisshdy,youmy contact-anonymously, if you wish-Peter Vasilenko, Ph.D., Chair of the University Committee on Mach Involving Human Subjects (UCRIHS) by phone: (517) 355-2180, talc (517)432-4503, e-mail: ucrihs@msu.edu, or reglda’ ma’l: 202 Olds hdl, East Lansing, MI 48824. Yorrassistmceinslpportinglhispiojectisgreatlywpreciated. PleaseconacteiflrerPeggyThelenor MchelleNicholscnifyouhaveanyspecificquestionsaboutdlisshidy. TheIlSDwiIlcontactyouregading fledbhbufionofdearuopriabdatabryoudishialsdbdassoonashisava'labb. Sincerely, Peggy Thelen Dr. Anne K Sodennal Graduate Student Professor of Family and Child Ecology College of Human Ecology College of Human Ecology Michigan State University Michigan State University (989) 463-6041 (517) 432-0692 thelenpe@msu.edu sodermaansuedu Illlichelle Nicholson SupervisorofEallyChildhood Education-School DevelopmentServices Ingham Intermediate School District (517) 244-1384 mnichols@inghamisd.org 177 APPENDIX E University Letter of Consent for Research 178 OFFICE OF RESEARCH ETHICS AND STANDARDS Irrhrerelty Committee ee Research Involving Human Subjects Mlchigm Sm University 202 Olds Hell Em Lsnslng. MI 48824 517135521” FAX: 51 71432-4503 : WNW/nulls E-Mail: Mew Wham-dim. WM MICHIGAN STATE u N r v E R s r T Y April 22. 2004 TO: Anne SODERMAN 13-H Human Ecology MSU RE: IRB# 04-260 CATEGORY: EXEMPT 1-1, 1-2 APPROVAL DATE: April 20, 2004 EXPIRATION DATE March 20, 2005 TITLE: Kindergarten Teacher Perceptions Regarding the Development of Young Children's Emerging Literacy Skills The University Committee on Research Involving Human Subjects' (UCRIHS) review of this project is complete and I am pleased to advise that the rights and welfare of the human subjects appear to be adequately protected and methods to obtain informed consent are appropriate. Therefore, the UCRIHS approved this project. - RENEWALS: UCRIHS approval is valid until the expiration date listed above. Projects continuing beyond this date must be renewed with the renewal form. A maximum of four such expedited renewals are possible. InvestigatOrs wishing to continue a project beyond that time need to submit a 5-year application for a complete review. REVISIONS: UCRIHS must review any changes in procedures involving human subjects. prior to initiation of the change. If this is done at the time of renewal, please include a revision form with the renewal. To revise an approved protocol at any other time during the year, send your written request with an attached revision cover sheet to the UCRIHS Chair, requesting revised approval and referencing the project's IRB# and title. Include in your request a description of the change and any revised instruments. consent forms or advertisements that are applicable. PROBLEMSICHANGES: Should either of the following arise during the course of the work. notify UCRIHS promptly: 1) problems (unexpected side effects, complaints, etc.) involving human subjects or 2) changes in the research environment or new information indicating greater risk to the human subjects than existed when the protocol was previously reviewed and approved. 'If we can be of further assistance, please contact us at (517) 355-2180 or via email: UCRIHS@msu.edu. Please note that all UCRIHS forms are located on the web: http:/Iwww.humanresearch.msu.edu Sincerely, Wfi Peter Vasilenko, Ph.D. UCRIHS Chair PV:rt 002 Peggy Thelen 4288 W. Jackson Rd. Alma. MI 48801 179 REFERENCES 180 References Adams, M. J. (1990). Beginning to read: Thinking and Ieaming about print. Cambridge, MA: Massachusetts Institute of Technology. Administration for Children and Families (n.d.). Compilation of the Head Start Act: Staff qualifications and development. Retrieved November 7, 2003, from http://www.acf.hhs.govlprogramslhsb/budgetlheadstartact.htm Administration for Children and Families (2002). Head Start history. Retrieved May, 8, 2003, from http:/lwwwacf.hhs.gov/programs/hsb/about/history.htm Administration for Children and Families (2003). Head Start program fact sheet. Retrieved May 8, 2003, from http://wwwacf.hhs.gov/proqrams/hsb/research/2003.htm Allington, R. L., & Cunningham, P. M. (1996). Schools that work: Where all children read and write. New York: Harper Collins. Almy, M. C. (1975). The early childhood educator at work. New York: McGraw- Hill, Inc. Anderson, A. B., 8. Stokes, S. J. (1984). Social and institutional influences on the development and practice of literacy. In H. Goelman, A. Oberg, 8. F. Smith (Eds), Awakening to literacy (pp. 2447). Portsmouth, NH: Heinernann. Anthony, J. L., Lonigan, C. J., 8. Driscoll, K. (2003). Phonological sensitivity A quasi-parallel progression of word structure units and cognitive operations. Reading Research Quarterly, 38, 4, 470-487. Au, K. H. (1981). Participation structures in a reading lesson with Hawaiian children: Analysis of a culturally appropriate instructional event. Anthropology and Education Quarterly, 11, 91-115. Au, K. H. (1993). Literacyinstruction in multicultural settings. Fort Worth, TX: Harcourt Brace. Au, K H., & Mason, J. M. (1981). Social organizational factors in Ieaming to read. Reading Research Quarterly, 17, 115-152. Baker, L., Sonnenschein, S., Serpell, R., Scher, D., et al. (1996). Early literacy at home: Children’s experiences and parents’ perspectives. The Reading Teacher, 50(1), 70-72. 181 Barnett, W. S. (1995). Long-term effect of early childhood programs on cognitive an school outcomes. The Future of Children, (3), 25-50. Baruch, Y. (1999). Response rate in academic studies—a comparative analysis. Human Relations, 52(4), 421 -439. Beals, D. E., & De Temple, J. M. (1993). Home contributions to early language and literacy development. In D. Leu & C. Kinzer (Eds), Examining central issues in literacy research, dreary, and practice. Forty-second yearbook of the National Reading Conference (pp. 207-215). Chicago: National Reading Conference. Berger, L., 8. Gunn, G. (2003). Challenging districts to ‘put reading first.” T. H. E. Journal, 30(10), $29-32. Bradley, L., & Bryant, P. E. (1983). Categorizing sounds and Ieaming to read: A causal connection. Nature, 301, 419-421. Bredekamp, S. (Ed.). (1987). Developmentally appropriate practice in early childhood programs serving children from birth through age 8. Washington, DC: National Association for the Education of Young Children. Bmdekamp, S., & Copple, C. (Eds.). (1997). Developmentally appropriate practice in early childhood programs. Washington DC: National Association for the Eduwtion of Young Children. Bronfenbrenner, U., & Morris, P. A. (1998). The ecology of developmental processes. In W. Damon (Series Ed.) 8. R. Lerner (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 1. Theoretical models of human development (5“ ed., pp. 993-1028). New York: Wiley 8. Sons. Bryant, D. M., Clifford, R. M., & Peisner, E. S. (1991). Best practices for beginners: Developmental appropriateness in kindergarten. American Education Research Journal, 28, 783-803. Burns, 8., Griffin, P., 8: Snow, C. E. (1999). Starting out right: A guide to promoting children’s reading success. Washington, DC: National Academy Press. Bus, A. G., van lJzendoom, M. H., & Pellegrini, A. D. (1995). Joint book reading makes for success in Ieaming to read: A meta-analysis in intergenerational transmission of literacy. Review of Educational Research, 65, 1-21. Byme, B. J. (1998). The foundation of literacy: The child’s acquisition of the alphabetic principle. East Sussex, UK: Psychology Press. 182 Caldwell, B. M., & Freyer, M. (1982). Day care and early edueation. In B. Spodek (Ed.), Handbook of research in early childhood (pp. 341-374). New York: Macmillan Publishing Company. Chall, J. S. (1967). Learning to read: The great debate. New York: McGraw-Hill. Chomsky, C. (1972). Write now, read later. In C. B. Cazden (Ed.), Language in early childhood education (pp.119-126). Washington, DC: National Association for the Education of Young Children. Christie, J., Enz, 8., & Vukelich, C. (1997). Teaching language and literacy: Preschool through the elementary grades. New York: Longman. Clay, M. M. (1966). Emergent reading behaviour. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. University of Auckland Library, New Zealand. Clay, M. M. (1976). Early childhood and cultural diversity in New Zealand. The Reading Teacher, 29, 333-342. Clay, M. M. (1979). Reading: The patterning of complex behavior (2":I ed.). Auckland, New Zealand: Heinemann. Cost, Quality, & Child Outcomes Study Team (1995). Cost, quality, and child outcomes in child care center, public report (2"d ed.). Denver, co: Economic Department, University of Colorado at Denver. Crain-Thoreson, C., & Dale, P. S. (1992). Do early talkers become early readers? Linguistic precocity, preschool language, and emergent literacy. Developmental Psychology, 28, 421 -429. Delgado-Gaitan, C., 8. Trueba, H. (1997). Crossing cultural borders: Education for immigrant families in America. New York: Falrner Press. DeVault, L. (2003). The tide is high, but we can hold on. Young Children, 58(6), 90-93. Dewey, J. (1990). The school and society, and, The child and the curriculum. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press. Dickinson, A (1999, November 8). Kinder Grind. Forget blocks, dress-up and show-and-tell. Five-year-olds are now being pushed to read. Are we asking too much too soon? Time, 154, 61-62. Dickinson, D. K., & DiGisi, L. L. (1998). The many rewards of a literacy-rich classroom. Educational Leadership, 55(6), 23-26. 183 Dickinson, D. K., & Tabors, P. 0. (1991). Early literacy: Linkages between home, school and literacy achievement at age five. Journal of Research in Childhood Education, 6(1), 30-46. Dowhower, S. L., 8. Beagle, K. G. (1998). The print environment in kindergartens: A study of conventional and holistic teachers and their classroom in three settings. Reading Research and Instruction, 37(3), 161- 190. Downs, R. B. (1978). Friedrich Froebel. Boston: Twayne Publishers. Duke, N. K. (2000). For the rich it’s richer“. Print experiences and environments offered to children in very low- and very-high socioeconomic status first-grade classrooms. American Educational Research Joumal, 37(2), 441 -478. Duke, N. K., 8. Bennett-Armistead, V. S. (2003). Reading & writing informational text in the primary grades. New York: Scholastic, Inc. Durkin, D. (1966). Children who read early: Two longitudinal studies. New York: Teachers College Press. Durkin, D. (1978). Teaching young children to read (2" ed.). Boston: Allyn and Bacon. Durkin, D. (1987). A classroom-observation study of reading instruction in kindergarten. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 2(3), 275-300. Edwards, D. (2000). Public factors that contribute to school readiness. Early Childhood Research and Practice, 1(2). Retrieved February 12, 2001, from http://ecrp.uiuc.edu/v1n2/print/edwards.htm| Edwards, P. A (1991). Fostering early literacy through parent coaching. In E. H. Hieger (Ed.), Literacy for a diverse society: Perspectives, practices, and policies (pp. 199- 212). New York: Teachers College Press Evans, M. A., Shaw, 0., 8. Bell, M. (2000). Home literacy activities and their influence on early literacy skills. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54(2), 65—75. Federal lnteragency Forum on Child and Family Statistics (2002). America’s Children 2002. Retrieved on August 10, 2003, from http:/Mww.childstats.govlac2002/http Fitzgerald, J., Spiegel, D. L., & Cunningham, J. W. (1991). The relationship between parental literacy level and perceptions of emergent literacy. Journal of Reading Behavior, 23, 191 -21 3. 184 Froebel, F. W. (1889). The education of man. New York: Appleton. Gadsden, V. L. (1998). Family cultures and literacy Ieaming. In J. Osborn & F. Lehr (Eds), Literacy for all. Issues in teaching and learning (pp. 32-53). New York: The Guilford Press. Galda, L., & Cullinan, B. (1991). Literature for literacy: What research says about the benefits of using trade books in the classroom. In J. Flood, J. Jensen, D. Lapp, & J. Squire, (Eds), Handbook of research on teaching the English Language Arts (pp.529-535). New York: Macmillan. Gans, R., Stendler, C., & Almy, M. (1952). Teaching young children in nursery school, kindergarten and the primary grades. Yonkers, NY: World Book Company. Gitomer, D. H., Latham, A S., & Ziomek, R. (1999). The academic quality of prospective teachers: The impact of admissions and licensure testing. Princeton, NJ: Educational Testing Service. Goertz, M., & Duffy, M. (2003). Mapping the landscape of high? stakes testing and accountability programs. Theory into Practice, 42(1), 4—1 1. Goldenberg, C. N., 8: Gallimore, R. (1995). Immigrant Latino parents’ values and beliefs about their children’s education: Continuities and discontinuities across cultures and generations. In P. Pintrich & M. Maehr (Eds), Culture, motivation, and achievement (pp. 183-227). Greenwich, CT: Jai Press. Goldenberg, C. N., Reese, L., 8 Gallimore, R. (1992). Effects of literacy materials from school on Latino children’s home experiences and reading achievement American Journal of Education, 100, 497-537. Goodman, Y. (1985). Kidwatching: Observing children in the classroom. In A. Jagger & M. Smith-Burke (Eds), Observing the language leamer. Newark, DE: lntemational Reading Association. (ERIC Document Reprbduction Services No. E0251 857) Goodman, Y. (1986). Children coming to know literacy. In W. Teale 8. E. Sulzby (Eds), Emergent literacy: Writing and reading. Norwood, NJ: Ablex. Graue, M. E. (1993). Ready for what? Constructing meanings of readiness for kindergarten. Albany, NY: State University of New York Press. Hall, N. (1991). Play and the emergence of literacy. In J. F. Christie (Ed.), Play and literacy development (pp. 3-25). Albany, NY: State University of New York Press. 185 Hall, S. L., 8 Moats, L. C. (1999). Straight talk about reading: How parents can make a difi'erence during the early years. Chiwgo: Contemporary Books. Hammer, C. S., Miccio, A. W., 8 Wagstaff, D. A. (2003). Home literacy experiences and their relationship to bilingual preschoolers. Developing English literacy abilities: An initial investigation. Language, speech, and hearing services in schools, 34(1), 20-30. Harste, J. C., 8 Burke, C. L. (1980) Examining instructional assumptions: The child as informant. Theoryinto Practice, 1(3), 170-178. Harste, J. C., Woodward, V. A, 8 Burke, C. L. (1984). Language stories and literacy lessons. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann. Hart, 8., 8 Risley, T. R. (1995). Meaningful difl'erences in the everyday experience of young American children. Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes Publishing Co. Heath, 8. B. (1982). What no bedtime story means: Narrative skills at home and school. Language in Society, 1(1), 49-76. Heath, S. B., (1983). Ways with words: Language, life, and work in communities and classrooms. New York: Cambridge University press. Hiebert, E. H. (1981). Developmental patterns and interrelationships of pre- school children’s point awareness. Reading Research Quarterly, 16, 236- 260. Hiebert, E. H., 8 Adams, C. S. (1987). Father’s and mother's perceptions of their preschool children’s emergent literacy. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 44, 2537. Hill, Patty Smith (1996). Kindergarten. In K. M. Paciorek 8 J. H. Munro (Eds), Sources: Notable selections in early childhood education (pp. 82-91). Guilford, CT: Brown 8 Benchmark Publishers. Holdaway, D. (1979). The foundations of literacy. Sydney, Australia: Ashton Scholastic. lntemational Reading Association and National Association for the Education of Young Children (2000). Position Statement Learning to read and write. Developmentally appropriate practice for young children. Washington, D. C. : NAEYC. Irwin, O. (1960). Infant speech: Effect of systematic reading of stories. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 3, 187-190. 186 Jehlen, A. (2004). Saving the day. NEA Today, 22, 24-37. Kaestle, C. F., 8 Vinovskis, M. A (1978). From apron strings to ABC’s: Parents, children and schooling in nineteenth century Massachusetts. In J. Demos 8 S. S. 800ka (Eds), Turning points: Historical and sociological essays on the family. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Katz, L. G. (1997, Fall). Tomorrow begins today: Implication from research. ERIC/EECE Newsletter, 9(2), 1-2. Kisker, E., Hofferth, 8., Phillips, 0., 8 Farquhar, E. (1991). A profile of child care: Early education and care in 1990. Washington, DC: U. S. Department of Education. Kostelnik, M. J., Soderman, A. K., 8 Whiren, A. P. (1999). Developmentally appropriate programs in early childhood education (2"‘I ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Menill. Kostelnik, M. J., Soderman, A. K., 8 Whiren, A. P. (2004). Developmentally appropriate curriculum: Best practices in early childhood education (3” ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson/Merrill Prentice Hall. Lareau, A. (1989). Home advantage: Social class and parental intervention in elementary education. New York: Falmer. Legislative Council, State of Michigan (2003). The revised school code. Act 451 of 1976. Retrieved on November 7, 2003, from httpjlwww.michiganlegislature.orgldocumentslmcllpdflmcl—380—1 531 .pdf Lively, K. (1998). States move to toughen standards for teacher-education programs. Chronicle of Higher Education, 44(47), A27-A28. Lomax, R. G., 8 McGee, L. M. (1987). Young children’s concepts about print and reading: Toward a model of word reading acquisition. Reading Research Quarterly, 22, 237-256. Lonigan, C. J., Dyer, S. M., 8 Anthony, J. L. (1996). The influence of the home literacy environment on the development of literacy skill in children from diverse racial and economic backgrounds. Paper presented at the Annual convention of the American Educational Research Association, New York. Lonigan, C. J., 8 Whitehurst, G. J. (1998). Golfing ready to read: Emergent literacy and family literacy. Bethesda, MA: National Institute of Child Health and Human Development; Washington, DC: Administration for Children and Families; Philadelphia, PA: Pew Charitable Tnlsts. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. ED450418). 187 Luke, A, 8 Kale, J. (1997). Learning through difference: Cultural practices in early childhood language socialization. In E. Gregory (Ed.), One child, many worlds (pp. 11-29). London, England: David Fulton Publishers. MacKinnon, P. (1959). Howdo children read? Montreal, Canada: Copp. Maclean, M., Bryant, P., 8 Bradley, L. (1987). Rhymes, nursery rhymes, and reading in early childhood. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 33, 255-281. Mason, 6., 8 Blanton, W. (1971). Story content for beginning reading instruction. Elementary English, 28, 793-796. Mason, J. M. (1992). Reading stories to children: A proposed connection to reading. In P. Gough, L. Ehir, 8 R. Treiman (Eds), Reading acquisition (pp. 215-242). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. McCarthey, S. J. (1997). Making the invisible more visible: Home literacy practices of middle-class and working-class families. Early Childhood and Care, 127, 179-189. McGee, L., 8 Richgels, D. (1996). Literacy's beginnings: Supporting young readers and writers. Boston: Allyn and Bacon. Michaels, S. (1981). “Sharing time”: Children’s nanative styles and differential access to literacy. Language in Society, 10(3), 423-442. Morgan, A L. (1987). The development of written language awareness in black preschool children. Journal of Reading Behavior (19), 1, 49—67. Michigan Department of Education (1996). Michigan Curriculum Framework. Michigan Department of Education. Michigan Department of Education (2004). Grade Level Content Expectations: Kindergarten English Language Arts. Retrieved on January 10, 2004, from www.michigan.gov/documents/ELA 00 (k) 87343 7.pdf Moon, C., 8 Wells, C. G. (1979). The influence of the home on Ieaming to read. Joumal of Research in Reading, 2, 53—62. Morrow, L. M. (1989). Literacy development in the early years. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Morrow, L. M. (1993). Literacy in the early years: Helping children read and write. Boston: Allyn and Bacon. 188 National Assessment of Educational Progress (1991). The 1989-1990 National assessment of reading and literature. Denver, CO: National Assessment of Educational Progress. National Association of State Directors of Teacher Education and Certification (NASDTEC) (1998). The NASDTEC manual on the preparation and certification of educational personnel. Dubuque, IA: Kendall/Hunt. National Center for Early Development and Learning (1997). Quality in child care centers. Early Childhood Research 8 Policy Briefs Fact Sheet, 1(1), 1-2. National Center for Education Statistics (2002). Digest of education statistics, 2001. Retrieved on May 8, 2003, from http://nces.edu.gov/puszOOZ/diqestZOO1/tables/dto43.asp National Research Council (1998). Preventing reading difliculties in young children. Washington, DC: National Academy Press. Neuman, S. 8., Copple, C., 8 Bredekamp, S. (2000). Leaming to read and write: Developmentally appropriate practices for young children. Washington, DC: National Association for the Education of Young Children. Neuman, S. 8., Hagedom, T., Celano, D., 8 Daly, P. (1995). Toward a collaborative approach to parent involvement in early education: A study of teenage mothers in an African-American community. American Educational Research Journal, 32, 801 -827. Neuman S. 8., 8 Roskos, K. (1997). Literacy knowledge in practice: Context of participation for young writers and readers. Reading Research Quarterly, 32(1), 10-32. Nielsen, D. C., 8 Monson, D. L. (1996). Effects of literacy environment on literacy development of kindergarten children. The Journal of Educational Research, 89, 259-71. Ninio, A. (1980). Picture-book reading in mother-infant dyads belonging to two sub-groups in Israel. Child Development, 51, 587-590. Ninio, A, 8 Bruner, J. (1978). The achievement and antecedents of labeling. Journal of Child Language, 5, 5—15. Ortiz, V. (1986). Reading activities and reading proficiency among Hispanic, Black, and White students. American Journal of Education, 95, 58-76. Palmer, J. S. (2001). Filly major thinkers on education: From Confircius to Dewey. New York: Rutledge. 189 Paratore, J. R., Melzi, G., 8 Krol-Sinclair, 8. (1999). What should we expect of family literacy? Newark, DE: lntemational Reading Association and The National Reading Conference. Paratore, J. R., Melzi, G., 8 KroI-Sinclair, 8. (2003). Learning about the literate lives of Latino families. In D. M. Barone 8 L. M. Morrow (Eds), Literacy and young children. Research-based practices (pp.101-118). New York: The Guilford Press. Peterson, D. S., Taylor, 8. M., 8 Hansen, R. (n.d.). Emergent literacy and oral language development. Retrieved September 21, 2003, from http://education.umn.edu/Cl/MREA/EmergentLitpdf Philips, S. U. (1983). Theinvisible culture. NewYork: Longman. Purcell-Gates, V. (1995). Other people’s words: The cycle of low literacy. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Purcell-Gates, V. (1996). Stories, coupons, and the TV Guide; Relationships between home literacy experiences and emergent literacy knowledge. Reading Research Quarterly, 31(4), 406-428. Purcell—Gates, V. (2000). Family Literacy. In M. L. Kamil, P. 8. Mosenthal, P. D. Pearson, 8 R. Barr (Eds), Handbook of reading research: Vol. 3 (pp. 853- 870). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Read, C. (1971). Preschool children’s knowledge of English phonology. Harvard Educational Review, 41(1), 1-34. Read, K. H., 8 Patterson, J. (1980). The nursery school and kindergarten. Human relationships and Ieaming. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Vlrrnston. Roskos, K. A, Christie, J. F., 8Richgels, D. J. (2003). Theessentialsofearly literacy instruction. Young Children, (58), 2, 52-60. Saluja, 6., Early, D. M., 8 Clifford, R. M. (2002, Spring). Demographic characteristics of early childhood teachers and structural elements of early care and education in the United States. Early Childhood Research 8 Practice, 4(1). Retrieved October 21, 2003, from http:/Ieclp.uiuc.edulv4nllsaluja.html Scarborough, H. 8., 8 Dobrich, W. (1994). On the efficacy of reading to preschoolers. Developmental Review, 14, 245-302. 190 Schickedanz, J. A. (1986). More than ABC’s: The early stages of reading and writing. Washington, DC: National Association for the Education of Young Children. Schieffelin, B. 8., 8 Cochran-Smith, M. (1984). Learning to read culturally Literacy before schooling. In H. Goelman, A. Oberg, 8 F. Smith (Eds), Awakening to literacy (pp. 3-23). Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann. Schmidt, P. R. (1998). Cultural conflict and struggle: Literacy Ieaming in a kindergarten program. New York: Peter Lang. Senechal, M., 8 Cornell, E. H. (1993). Vocabulary acquisition through shared reading experiences. Reading Research Quarterly, 28, 360-375. Senechal, M., LeFevre, J., Hudson, E., 8 Lawson, E. P. (1996). Knowledge of storybooks as a predictor of young children’s vocabulary. Journal of Educational Psychology, 88, 520-536. Senechal, M., LeFevre, J., Thomas, E. M., 8 Daley, K. K. (1998). Differential effects of home literacy experiences on the development of oral and written language. Reading Research Quarterly, 13, 96-116. Shapiro, M. S. (1983). Child’s garden: The kindergarten movement from Froebel to Dewey. University Park, PA: The Pennsylvania State University Press. Snow, C. E., Barnes, W., Chandler, J., Goodman, l., 8 Hemphill, L. (1991). Unfulfilled expectations: Home and school influences in literacy. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Snow, C., Burns, M. 8., 8 Griffin, P. (Eds). (1998). Preventing reading difliculties in young children. Washington, DC: National Academy Press. Soderman, A K, Gregory, K. M., 8 O’Neill, L. T. (1999). Scafl'olding emergent literacy. A child-centered approach for preschool through grade 5. Boston: Allyn and Bacon. Soderman, A. K, Gregory, K. M., 8 McCarty, L. T. (2005). Scafl'olding emergent literacy. A child-centered approach for preschool through grade 5 (2"d ed). Boston: Allyn and Bacon. Sonnenschein, S., Brody, G., 8 Munstennan, K. (1996). The influence of family beliefs and practices on children’s early reading development. In L. Baker, P. Afflerbach, 8 D. Reinking (Eds), Developing engaged readers in school and home communities (pp. 3—20). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. 191 Spodek, B. (1996). The kindergarten: A retrospective and contemporary view. In K. M. Paciorek 8 J. H. Munro (Eds), Sources: Notable selections in early childhood education (pp. 92-103). Guilford, CT: Brown 8 Benchmark Stahl, S. A, 8 McKenna, M. C. (2000, April 25). The concurrent development of phonological awareness, word recognition, and spelling. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Education Research Association. Retrieved September 21, 2003, from htthlciera.orgllibrarylarchivel2001-07 [200107.htm Stahl, S. A, 8 Munay, B. A. (1994). Defining phonological awareness and its relationship to early reading. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86, 221-234. Stanovich, K. E., Cunningham, A. E., 8 Cramer, B. (1984). Assessing phonological awareness in kindergarten children: Issues of task comparability. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 38, 175-190. Stephens, D., 8 Clyde, J. A. (1985). Curriculum as theory driven: Insights from six classrooms. In J. C. Halste 8 D. Stephens (Eds), Toward practical theory: A state of practice assessment of reading comprehension instructions. Bloomington, IN: Indiana University, Language Education Department. Stevenson, H. W., 8 Newman, R. S. (1986). Long-tenn prediction of achievement and attitudes in mathematics and reading. Child Development, 57, 646-659. Strickland, C. E. (1982). Paths not taken: Seminal models of early childhood education in Jacksonian America. In 8. Spodek (Ed.), Handbook of research in early childhood (pp. 321-340). New York: Macmillan Publishing Company. Strickland, D. S., 8Morrow, L. M. (1989). Emergingliteracy: Youngchildren leamtoreadandwrite. Newark, DE: lntemationalReadingAssociation. Sulzby, E., 8 Edwards, P. A (1993). The role of parents in supporting literacy development of young children. In B. Spodak 8 O. N. Salacho (Eds), Language and literacy in early childhood education (pp. 156-177). New York: Teachers College Press. Sulzby, E., 8 Teale, W. H. (1991). Emergent literacy. In R. Barr, M.L. Kamil, P. Mosenthal, 8 P. D. Pearson (Eds), Handbook of reading research (Vol. 2, pp.727-757). New York: Longman. Taylor, D. (1 998). Family literacy: Young children Ieaming to read and write (2" ed.). Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann. 192 Taylor, D., 8 Dorsey-Gaines, C. (1988). Growing up literate: Learning from inner-city families. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann. Taylor, N. E., Blum, I. H., 8 Logsdon, D. M. (1986). The development of written language awareness: Environmental aspects and program characteristics. Reading Research Quarterly, 21(2), 132-149. Taylor, R. L. (1995). Functional uses of reading and shared literacy activities in Icelandic homes: A monograph in family literacy. Reading Research Quarterly, 30, 194-219. Teale, W. H. (1978). Positive environments for learning to read: What studies of early readers tell us. Language Arts, 55, 922-932. Teale, W. H. (1981). Parents reading to their children: What we know and need to know. Language Arts, 58(8), 902-912. Teale, W. H. (1986). Home background and young children’s literacy development In W. H. Teale 8 E. Sulzby (Eds), Emergent literacy: Writing andreading (pp. 173-206). Norwood, NH: Ablex. Teale, W. H. (1995). Young children and reading: Trends across the twentieth century. Journal of Education, 177(3), 95—125. Teale, W. H.,8Sulzby, E. (Eds). (1986). Emergentliteracy: Writingand reading. Norwood, NJ: Ablex. Teale, W. H., 8 Sulzby, E. (1989). Emergent literacy: New perspectives. In D. S. Strickland 8 L. M. Morrow (Eds), Emerging literacy: Young children learn to read and write (pp. 1-15). Newark, DE: lntemational Reading Assoc. Thelen, P. (2002). Kindergarten Readiness Skill Survey. Kindergarten and Head Start teachers: Do they agree on readiness skills and abilities? Unpublished report for the Lansing School District, Lansing, MI. Thomas, R. M. (2000). Comparing theories of child development Stamford, CT: Wadsworthfl'homson Learning. United States Census Bureau (1995). Urban and rural definitions. Retrieved September 8, 2004, from http:/lwww.census.govlpopulationlcensusdatalurdef.txt. United States Census Bureau (2001). The Hispanic population in the United States. Retrieved July 30, 2003, from httpleww.censusgovlpopulationlsocdemolhispaniclp20-535.pd. 193 United States Census Bureau (2003). Statistical Abstract of the United States. Retrieved July 30, 2003, from http://www.census.gov/prod/2003publeZstatab/labor.pdf United States Department of Commerce, Bureau of the Census (2001). Historical statistics of the United States; Colonial times to 1970; Current population reports, Series P-20, various years; and cunent population survey, unpublished data. Retrieved July 30, 2003, from http'Jl www.census.govlpopuIationldocumentationltwps0056lhtmI United States Department of Education (2002). Introduction: No child left behind. Retrieved April 22, 2003, from http://www.nochildleftbehind.gov/next/overview/index/htmI United States Department of Health and Human Services (n.d.). Head Start Child Outcorrre Framework. Washington, D. C. : USDHHS. Valdes, G. (1996). Con respeto: Bridging the gap between culturally diverse families and schools. New York: Teachers College Press. Volk, D. (1997). Questions in lessons. Anthropology and Education Quarterly, 28, 22-49. Wagner, R. K., 8 Torgesen, J. K. (1987). The nature of phonological processing and its causal role in the acquisition of reading skills. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 192-212. Wagner, R. K, Torgesen, J. K, 8 Rashotte, C. A. (1994). Development of reading-related phonological processing abilities: New evidence of bi- directional causality from a latent variable longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 30, 73-87. Walker, G. H., 8 Kuerrbitz, l. E. (1979). Reading to preschoolers as an aid to a successful beginning reading. Reading Improvement, 16, 149-154. Wells, G. (1985). Language development in the preschool years. New York: Cambridge University Press. White, M. P. (1982). The relation between socioeconomic status and academic achievement. Psychological Bulletin, 91, 461 -481. Whitehurst, G. J., 8 Lonigan, C. J. (1998). Child development and emergent literacy. Child Development, 68, 848-872. 194 Whitehurst, G. J., 8 Lonigan, C. J. (2001). Emergent literacy: Development from prereaders to readers. In S. B. Neuman 8 K. K. Dickinson (Eds), Handbook of early literacy research (pp. 11-29). New York: Guilford Press. Winsor, P. J., 8 Pearson, P. D. (1992). Children at risk: Their phonemic awareness development in holistic instruction (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. ED345209). Xu, 8. H. (1999). Reexamining continuities and discontinuities: Language minority children’s home and school literacy experiences. National Reading Conference Yearbook, 48, 224-237. Xu, S. H. (2003). The learner, the teacher, the text, and the context. Sociocultural approaches to early literacy instruction for English language Ieamers. In D. M. Barone 8 L. M. Morrow (Eds), Literacy and young children. New York: Guilford. Yaden, D. B., Rowe, D. W., 8 MacGillivray, L. (2000). Emergent literacy: A matter (polyphony) of perspectives. In M. L. Kamil, P. B. Mosenthal, P. D. Pearson, 8 R. Barr (Eds), Handbook of reading research: Vol. 3 (pp.425-445). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Yatvin, J., Weaver, 0., 8 Garan, E. (2003). Reading first: Cautions and recommendations. Language Arts, 81(1), 28-35. 195 ‘INI'VI'HSII T LIURARIF 'r mumrmu ‘il-‘ATE _ r r I r I . I “ ‘ ‘ IIIIIIIIIHIIII 3 1293 02504 0399