LIBRARIES MICHIGAN STATE UNIVERSITY EAST LANSING, MICH 48824-1048 This is to certify that the thesis entitled CHILDREN’S SOCIAL COMPETENCE AND THE DEVELOPMENTAL APPROPRIATENESS OF THEIR CLASSROOM ENVIRONMENT presented by Erica Celeste Poindexter has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for the M A degree in Child Development 7 .1, I .i’ l , ~"t '/ A - J- ' Major rJrofessor’s Signature I l/ . 4%; ¢ I ’ I Date MSU is an Affirmative Action/Equal Opportunity Institution PLACE IN RETURN BOX to remove this checkout from your record. TO AVOID FINES return on or before date due. MAY BE RECALLED with earlier due date if requested. DATE DUE DATE DUE DATE DUE 090900782£389 8/01 Ct/CIRC/DaIeDuepBSvaS fi—— _.V CHILDREN’S SOCIAL COMPETENCE AND THE DEVELOPMENTAL APPROPRIATENESS OF THEIR CLASSROOM ENVIRONMENT By Erica Celeste Poindexter Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF ARTS Department of Family and Child Ecology 2004 Professor Anne K. Soderman ABSTRACT CHILDREN’S SOCIAL COMPETENCE AND THE DEVELOPMENTAL APPROPRIATENESS OF THEIR CLASSROOM ENVIRONMENT By Erica Celeste Poindexter The purpose of this study was to investigate the relationship between teachers’ developmentally appropriate practices observed in kindergarten classrooms and children’s social competency skills. Using the Assessment of Practices in Early Elementary Classrooms (APEEC) and the Pre-K/Kindergarten Social Competency Scale (PK/K-S), this study examined the influences of teachers’ physical environment, instructional context, and social context on children’s levels of social competence. One hundred and sixty-six kindergarten children and 11 kindergarten teachers in an urban, Midwest school district participated in this study. As would be expected, a paired t-test indicated a positive change in children’s social competency skills over the course of the academic school year. A multiple regression analysis showed a significant relationship between children’s social competency scores in November and teachers’ scores on the APEEC. However, a second analysis indicated there were no statistically significant relationships between children’s social competence and teachers’ scores on the APEEC in May. It is believed that teachers’ problematic scoring of the PK/K-S may have contributed to this non-significant relationship. Implications for future studies strongly persuade researchers to have more than one observer complete the PK/K-S. It is also suggested that additional demographic information for each child’s SES status, temperamental style, and disability level be collected in order to help identify which individual characteristics may be related to social competence. To all of the people who believed in me and never lost faith that I would achieve success. iii ACKNOWLEDGEMENT First and foremost, I would like to express my sincere gratitude and love to my family. Without them, I would not have made it this far. Their support, guidance, love, and motivation has lifted me and inspired me beyond belief. Daddy, thank you for believing in me and supporting me. Through all of this, you have always been a positive voice of reason. Clifford, thank you for the late nights, motivational speeches, last minute e-mails, and endless laughs. Shawn, thanks for never giving up on me, and never letting me give up on myself. Your harsh but loving taste of reality was exactly what I needed to stay focused. Mom, thanks for giving me the gift of patience, because I needed it now more than ever. Thanks for sharing your love for children, and uplifting smiles. Although you are not physically here, I know you have never left my side. Thanks for continuing to shine your rays of hope, through those somewhat cloudy days. To each of you...THANK YOU and I LOVE YOU!!! Second, I would like to my mentor and advisor, Dr. Anne K. Soderman, who envisioned my path of success and directed and guided me with undoubted hope through my journey at Michigan State University. Thank you for envisioning my future and seeing me through. You are truly a blessing and an inspiration. I would also like to thank the members of my committee, Dr. Tom Luster and Dr. Darrell Meece, for their support, patience, and encouragement. Your comments and assistance helped to carry me through this. iv Lastly, I would like to thank all of the faculty members at the MSU Child Development Lab that supported me and guided me through all of this. Your kindness and expertise has profoundly impacted my life, as well as enhanced my passion for working with young children. Thanks for believing in me. TABLE OF CONTENTS _P_ag§ LIST OF TABLES ................................................................................. viii LIST OF FIGURES ................................................................................. ix CHAPTER 1 INTRODUCTION ................................................................................... 1 Purpose of the Study ................................................................................. 3 Significance of the Study ........................................................................... 3 Conceptual Models .................................................................................. 8 Research Questions ................................................................................. 10 Conceptual and Operational Definitions ......................................................... 11 Assumptions .......................................................................................... 12 Summary ............................................................................................. 12 CHAPTER II . REVIEW OF LITERATURE ...................................................................... 14 Social Competence ......................................................................... 14 Influences on Social Competence .......................................................... 14 Developmentally Appropriate Practices (DAP) ......................................... 23 Components of DAP ........................................................................ 25 Summary .................................................................................... 29 CHAPTER III METHODS ........................................................................................... 30 Research Design ............................................................................ 3O Participants .................................................................................. 30 Research Questions ........................................................................ 31 Measures .................................................................................... 31 Pre-K/Kindergarten Social Competency Scale (PK/K-S) ................... 31 Assessment of Practices in Early Elementary Classrooms (APEEC) ...... 32 Data Collection Procedure ................................................................ 34 Data Analysis ............................................................................... 35 Limitations .................................................................................. 36 CHAPTER IV RESULTS ............................................................................................ 38 Reliability-intemal Consistency of PK/K-S ............................................. 38 RQl: Do teacher ratings of children’s social competence vary depending on demographic variables, such as gender and ethnicity? ...................... 39 RQ2: Do children’s social competency skills change over the course of the school year? .......................................................................... 40 vi RQ3: How do kindergarten children’s social competency abilities, rated in November and May, relate to their teacher’s physical environment scores obtained on the Assessment of Practices in Early Elementary Classrooms (APEEC)? ........................................................................... 41 RQ4: How do kindergarten children’s social competency abilities, rated in November and May, relate to their teacher’s instructional context scores obtained on the APEEC? ......................................................... 42 RQS: How do kindergarten children’s social competency abilities, rated in November and May, relate to their teacher’s social context scores obtained on the APEEC? .......................................................... 43 RQ6: How do kindergarten children’s social competency abilities, rated in November and May, relate to their teacher’s total score obtained on the APEEC? ........................................................................... 43 Summary of Data ........................................................................... 44 CHAPTER V DISCUSSION ....................................................................................... 48 Implications for Practice and Future Research ......................................... 54 APPENDIX A: Pre-K/Kindergarten Social Competency Scale (PK/K-S) ............. -.....56 REFERENCES ...................................................................................... 58 vii LIST OF TABLES Table 1. Gender and Age of Children ........................................................... 30 Table 2. Ethnicity of Children .................................................................... 31 Table 3. Reliability Analysis of PK/K-S ........................................................ 38 Table 4. T-Test for differences between males and females in November and May......39 Table 5. Results of a One-Way ANOVA analysis of variance for ethnicity in children’s PK/K-S scores ........................................................................ 40 Table 6. Paired T-Test: Social competency scores in November and May ................. 41 Table 7. Multiple Regression Analysis: Physical environment as a predictor of kindergarten children’s social competence ....................................... 42 Table 8. Multiple Regression Analysis: Instructional context as a predictor of kindergarten children’s social competence.......................................42 Table 9. Multiple Regression Analysis: Social context as a predictor of kindergarten children’s social competence ....................................................... 43 Table 10. Multiple Regression Analysis: Total scores on the APEEC as a predictor of kindergarten children’s social competence ....................................... 44 Table 11. Summary of Findings ................................................................. 46 Table 12. PK/K-S measured in November ...................................................... 50 Table 12. PK/K-S measured in November ...................................................... 51 viii LIST OF FIGURES Figure 1. The Ecology of Social Competence ................................................... 9 Figure 2. The Ecology of Social Competence As It Applies To This Study ................ 10 ix CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION Social competence is an evaluative term defined as the ability to function effectively in society appropriate to your age (Kostelnik, Soderman, & Whiren, 2004). More specifically, it refers to the overall quality and adequacy of the behavior one displays in his or her interactions with others. This term reflects the judgments people make about behavior, especially in regard to teacher expectations, peer acceptance, social-emotional regulation, and communication (Landy, 2002). In order to achieve social competence, one must possess the skills that meet the demands of the people with whom he or she interacts with on a daily basis. (Knapczyk & Rodes, 1996; McFall, 1982). It is the development of these skills that have been linked to children’s levels of happiness, success, popularity, and satisfaction. It is also stated that children who learn to develop these positive social relations have more positive social relationships, which in turn allows them to achieve greater school success (Pellegrini & Glickman, 1990; Bemdt & Keefe, 1995; Kostelnik et al., 2002). In the classroom environment, caregivers and teachers are thought to impact children’s social competence in a variety of ways. Examples include: how they instruct children; how they model social behaviors and attitudes; how they plan the physical environment; how they formulate routines; and how they discipline children (Kostelnik et al., 2002). In fact, the way in which these strategies are implemented could result in a social learning environment that enhances or inhibits children’s developing attitudes and behaviors associated with social competence (Kostelnik et al., 2002). More specifically, it can be concluded that the developmental appropriateness of the environment is extremely important in relation to enhancing children’s social competence. The term “developmentally appropriate practice” or DAP refers to the decisions made about the well-being and education of young children on the basis of three important sources of knowledge (Bredekamp & Copple, 1997; Kostelnik, Soderman, & Whiren, 2004): what teachers know about how children develop and learn; what teachers know about the strengths, needs, and interests of individual children; and what they know about the social and cultural contexts in which children live. According to the National Association for the Education of Young Children (NAEYC), there are certain distinct attributes that can be found in all developmentally appropriate programs. For example, all DAP programs provide “a safe and nurturing environment that promotes the physical, social, emotional, aesthetic, intellectual, and language development of each child while being sensitive to the needs and preferences of families” (Bredekamp & Copple, 1997, p.8). Furthermore, all DAP programs are based on the notion that children learn by actively constructing their own knowledge through interacting with peers, adults, and materials, with educational decisions being guided by questions relating to age, individual, social, and cultural experiences (Jones & Gullo, 1999). In fact, the core idea behind DAP is that “the child is the basic unit of the curriculum” (Jambunanthan, Butts, & Pierce, 1999, p. 167). Incorporating these developmental guidelines into the teaching practices used in elementary classrooms were found to lead to fewer negative social behaviors, better problem solving skills, and more cooperation in children than those enrolled in a more traditional classroom (Kostelnik, Soderman, & Whiren, 2004). The Purpose of the Study The pugpose of this study is to investigate the relationship that exists between teachers’ developmentally appropriate practices observed in kindergarten classrooms and children’s social competency skills. More specifically, this study will determine if certain aspects of the classroom, including the physical environment, instructional context, and social context relate to children’s social competency. Significance of Study The term DAP, adopted by NAEYC (Bredekamp & Copple, 1997), was specifically designed to raise the quality of curriculum and program practices for all young children (Raines & Johnson, 2003). Particularly, it was designed to ensure that the child was the basic unit of the curriculum (Jambunanthan, Burts, & Pierce, 1999). All aspects of the program were to revolve around the physical, social, emotional, and intellectual needs of the child, while also ensuring that the child was in a safe and organized environment (Bredekamp & Copple, 1997). Several studies were designed to assess the effects associated with DAP. Specifically, studies pertaining to the developmental appropriateness of kindergarten teachers’ beliefs and practices and those examining the social and academic effects of DAP have concluded that curricular approaches that incorporate developmentally appropriate practices have led to positive educational outcomes (Jones & Gullo, 1999; Charlesworth, Hart, Burts, & Hernandez, 1991; Charlesworth, Hart, Burts, Thomasson, Mosley, & Fleege, 1993; Burts, Hart, Charlesworth, DeWolf, Ray, Mauel, & Fleege, 1993; Maroon, 1992; Sherman & Mueller, 1996). In terms of gender and ethnicity, studies have shown that DAP has been shown to have positive effects on academic and behavioral outcomes, as well as alleviating stress-related behaviors of lower socioeconomic, non-mainstream, non-European, and male students (Hart, Burts, Durland, Charlesworth, DeWolf, & F leege, 1998; Bloch, 1992; Carmella, 1997; Powell & Lawton, 1992; Jipson, 1991; Kessler, 1991a, 1991b; Kessler & Swadener, 1992; Lubeck, 1994; Ludlow & Berkeley, 1994; O’Loughlin, 1992; Walsh, 1991). Despite all of the reports stating the positive effects related to DAP, much work still remains in order to increase the implementation of DAP and advocate the importance of it in relation to the development of young children. In fact, developmentally appropriate practices are still not the norm in early childhood education programs (Dunn & Kontos, 1997; Raines & Johnson, 2003). Since it is unclear why the majority of early childhood programs are not implementing DAP, one of the intentions of this study is to further examine the usefulness of DAP. In addition to determining its importance, this study is also designed to add to the literature available regarding the relationship that exists between developmentally appropriate practices used in kindergarten classrooms and children’s social competence. Much research exists linking DAP with socioemotional and cognitive development; however, fewer studies exist that examine the relationship between DAP and social development. Based on this information and the scarcity of research found pertaining to this issue, there is a great need to further study the outcomes that developmentally appropriate practices has on the development of young children, especially in regards to social competence. Conceptual Framework Human ecology theory was chosen as an appropriate framework for analyzing the relationship that exists between kindergarten children’s level of social competence and the developmental appropriateness of their classroom environment. The basis of human ecology theory suggests that the quality of one’s life depends mainly on the interaction and interdependence that humans have with their environment (Bubolz & Sontag, 1993). Several characteristics that help to describe the quality of one’s life include but are not limited to: safety, support, aesthetics, and satisfaction (Bubolz & Sontag, 1993). Of interest in this study was the developmental appropriateness of the classroom context and its relationship to kindergarteners’ developing social competence. In human ecology theory, the well-being of individuals cannot be considered apart from the well-being of the whole ecosystem (Bulboz & Sontag, 1993). In other words, one must closely examine the surrounding environments in which the individual interacts, in order to identify their existing relationships. More specifically, to gain a better understanding of behavior and development in relation to one’s environment, an ecological analysis should be conducted based on Bronfenbrenner’s four interdependent organizational systems (1979; 1989; Griffore & Phenice, 2001). These systems include, the microsystem, the mesosystem, the exosystem, and the macrosystem. For the purposes of this study, the microsystem of the classroom was chosen as the unit of analysis, consisting of a pattern of activities, roles, and interpersonal relations experienced by the developing persons (in this study, the children and the teachers) in a given setting (in this study, the classroom) with particular physical, instructional, and material characteristics, that may limit or promote development (in this study, social competence) (Bronfenbrenner, 1993; Thomas, 2000). Evidence shows that children are significantly influenced by the microsystem in which they live, especially in terms of social competence (Kostelnik, et a1. 2002). The family unit, which is the primary system in which children interact, provides them with their earliest social relationships that prepare them for the social world. It is the settings located in the microsystems beyond the home, that peers, relatives, caregivers and teachers provide additional opportunities for social learning (Kostelnik et al. 2002). In fact, a significant amount of social learning occurs at school, where children interact with peers of the same age. Through peer relationships, children learn concepts of reciprocity and equity; consequences of their behavior for both themselves and others; the appropriateness of their actions; and how to modify their behavior according to the feedback they receive from their peers. Teachers also have a crucial role in promoting children’s social competence (Clarke-Stewart, Gruber, & Fitzgerald, 1994; Kostelnik et al., 2002). Strategies related to how they instruct, formulate routines, discipline, plan the physical environment, and model social behaviors and attitudes, either enhance or inhibit children’s developing attitudes and behaviors associated with social competence (Kostelnik et al., 2002). Based on this theoretical framework, a conceptual model was designed to show the ecological development of social competency based on the interactions that the children have within their microsystems. Figure 1 illustrates the broad range of biological and environmental influences that contribute to the development of children’s social competence. Figure 2, on the other hand, illustrates the conceptual model of the actual influences of social competence that were examined in this study. This figure looks specifically at how the teacher and classroom environment influences children’s social competence. Figure 1: The Ecology of Social Competence F Emu mmcg W Provides children with: F W 1 - Their earliest social Impacts children’s social relationships competence through: (Attachments) I Instruction I Manners, views, beliefs, - Interaction: modeling & ideas deemed appropriate social k acceptable by society J \ behavior& attitudes J (INDIXQQAL INFLQEQEN 9"” Innate duracteristics impacting the development of: - Temperament - Communication skills \ J (mam,mngm\ Provides a safe environment where children: - Build social relationships and peer sociability ' Enhance language development \ J ’ F 2m mm \ Promotes understanding of: \\\\\\\\\\\\\\\\\\\\\\\\\\\\\\\\\\\\\\\\\\w ' “WWW emotions, motives, & intentions - The consequences resulting fi'om one’s behavior Key: On Page 9 / .i gt; é ( Figure 2: The Ecology of Social Competence As It Applies To This Study Impacts children’s social competence through: I Instruction I Interaction: modeling appropriate social behavior& attitudes I Planning the physical k environment / TEACHE LUEN E\ j _ E) =Unit of Analysis MT= Connection Research Questions This study will address the following research questions: Research Question I : Do teacher ratings of children’s social competence vary depending on demographic variables, such as gender and ethnicity? Research Question 2: Do children’s social competency skills change over the course of the school year? Research Question 3 .' How do kindergarten children’s social competency abilities, rated in November and May, relate to their teacher’s physical environment scores obtained on the Assessment of Practices in Early Elementary Classrooms (APEEC)? Research Question 4: How do kindergarten children’s social competency abilities, rated in November and May, relate to their teacher’s instructional context scores obtained on the APEEC? Research Question 5 .' How do kindergarten children’s social competency abilities, rated in November and May, relate to their teacher’s social context scores obtained on the APEEC? Research Question 6: How do kindergarten children’s social competency abilities, rated in November and May, relate to their teacher’s total score obtained on the APEEC? 10 Conceptual and Operational Definitions Social competence. Generally, social competence can be defined as the ability to function effectively in society appropriate to your age (Kostelnik, Soderman, & Whiren, 2004). However, definitions of social competence tend to vary from one society to another. In the United States, children who are socially competent are viewed as being responsible, independent, friendly, cooperative, purposeful, and self-controlled (Baumrind, 1995; Kostlenik, et al. 2002). Based on these characteristics, this study conceptually defines children’s social competence as the overall quality and adequacy of the behavior one displays in his or her interactions in regards to teacher expectations, peer acceptance, social-emotional regulation, and communication (Landy, 2002; Knapczyk & Rodes, 1996). For this study, social competence is operationally defined as teacher ratings of children’s social competency skills as scored on the Pre-K/Kindergarten Social Competency Scale (PK/K-S) (Soderman, 2002). Developmental appropriateness of the classroom environment. The developmental appropriateness of the classroom environment is conceptually defined by the National Association for the Education of Young Children (N AEYC) as an environment in which teachers recognize the importance of, and integrate the following practices into their classroom: (1) knowledge about children and how they develop and learn, (2) knowledge about individual children in their group, (3) knowledge about the social and cultural context in which children live and learn, and (4) knowledge about curriculum planning, recognizing that the curriculum should be integrated, including knowledge and skills from all domains of development (Bredekamp and Copple, 1997). 11 For this study, developmental appropriateness of the children’s classroom environment is operationally defined as an observational rating of kindergarten teacher’s performance as scored on the Assessment of Practices in Early Elementary Classrooms (APEEC) (Hemmeter et al., 2001). The scales used in the APEEC were categorized according to: physical environment, instructional context, and social context. Assumptions The assumptions on which this study is based are: 1. The scores given by the teachers on the PK/K-S are valid. 2. The scores received on the PK/K-S will vary by gender and ethnicity. 3. The scores given by the observers on the APEEC are valid. 4. Children come to school with varying skills of social competence. 5. Teachers have different perceptions of a developmentally appropriate classroom. 6. The Pre-K/K-S measures social competence. 7. Children’s interactions outside of the classroom also contribute to their level of social competence. 8. Throughout the course of the school year, children’s social competency scores can change both positively and negatively. Summary This study was designed to analyze the relationship that exists between children’s social competence in November and May and the developmental appropriateness of their classroom environment. The human ecological theory used as the conceptual framework for this study proposes that in order to understand children’s behavior and development, children must be examined within the environment in which they interact. Based on this 12 framework, a conceptual model was designed to illustrate the influences within children’s classroom environments that contributes to their level of social competence. More specifically, the model that applies to this study examined how children’s interactions with their teacher and their classroom environment influenced their development of social competence. The following chapter includes a literature review of related research. Information pertaining to the methodology is located in Chapter III. The results of this study will be presented in Chapter IV and discussions and implications for practice and for future research will be presented in Chapter V. 13 CHAPTER II LITERATURE REVIEW Social Competence The goal of this research is to determine how social competency relates to the developmental appropriateness of the classroom environment. Social competence is an evaluative term defined as the ability to function effectively in society appropriate to your age (Kostelnik, Soderman, & Whiren, 2004). This term reflects the judgments people make about behavior, especially in regards to teacher expectations, peer acceptance, social-emotional regulation, and communication (Landy, 2002). Other behaviors associated with social competence include: adopting social values, developing a sense of personal identity, acquiring interpersonal skills, learning how to regulate personal behavior in accordance with societal expectations, planning and making decisions, and developing cultural competence (Kostelnik, et al. 2002). Past research has concluded that social competence is an important asset capable of successfully facilitating a child’s adjustment to school (Bates, Luster, & Vandenbelt, 2003). However, as additional research will show, a child’s success rate also depends heavily on the existence and quality of environmental factors such as peers, family, and teacher influences. Other individual characteristics that children possess also contribute to their level of competence. Research specifically targets biological factors such as temperament, communication skills, and the status of one’s health, as potential genetic influences. Influences of Social Competence Peer Influences. One influence that helps to determine a children’s level of social competence is the relationship they have with their peers. The more children interact 14 with others of the same age group at school, the more social learning occurs (Kostelnik et al. 2002). Through peer relationships, children learn concepts of reciprocity and equity; conflict resolution and problem solving; an understanding of other’s thoughts, emotions, motives, and intentions; and an evaluation of the appropriateness of their actions (Kostelnik et al., 2002; Santrock, 2000a). In fact, there is a general consensus in the literature that making friends, keeping friends, and being friends with others are important indices of social competence during childhood (Vaughn, Colvin, Azria, Caya, & Krzysik, 2001; Bemdt, 1989, 1996; Bukowski, Newcomb & Hartup, 1996; Hartup, 1996a; Hartup & Stevens, 1997; Parker & Asher, 1993; Parker, Rubin, Price, & DeRosier, 1995; Vaughn, Azria, Krzysik, Caya, Bost, Newell, & Kazura, 2000). Furthermore, interactions with friends tend to be associated with social competence in ways that interactions with non-fiiend acquaintances are not (Vaughn et al. 2001; Azmitia & Montgomery, 1993; Hartup, 1996b; Ladd & Kochenderfer, 1996; Newcomb & Bagwell, 1995, 1996). Research shows that children who lack friends and acceptance by peers are less likely to experience social success, and adjust well to school (Kostelnik et al., 2002; Ladd & Kochendefer, 1996). Studies have found that kindergarteners whom their peers victimized, or who were aggressive toward others, had more school- adjustment problems, including school difficulties, delinquency, and drug abuse. On the other hand, children who entered school with fiiends, who were well liked, and who were able to make and sustain new friendships, were noted as being able to initiate positive relationships with teachers. These children tend to feel more positive about school, participate in school more, and have higher achievements than children not as friendly or able to establish friendships (Denham et al. 2003; Gagnon, Craig, Tremblay, Zhou & 15 Vitaro, 1995; Haapasalo & Tremblay, 1994; Ladd & Kochenderfer, 1996; Tremblay, Pagani-Kurtz, Masse, Vitaro, & Pihl, 1995). Overall, studies find that children who are able to establish positive peer relationships are more likely to be successful in school and in other aspects of life (Kostelnik et al., 2002; Birch & Ladd, 1996; Kontos & Wilcox- Herzog, 1997). Family Influences. The primary function of a family, no matter the type, is to be responsible for providing children with physical needs, nurturance, and skills for socialization (Kostelnik et al., 2002; Goldsmith, 1999). As children’s first teachers, families provide children with their earliest social relationships, models for behaviors and roles, a framework for values and beliefs, and intellectual stimulation (Kostelnik et-al., 2002; Bobbitt & Paolucci, 1986). They also introduce children to environments outside of the home, like school. Essentially, it is the continued interactions that the family has with these environments (i.e. school) that make the difference in children’s development. In a study conducted by Taylor and Machida (1994), studying the contributions of parent involvement in Head Start children’s school adjustment, findings showed that supportive personal relationships between children and their parents helped children better adapt to the school environment. Another study found that family support was a predictor of children’s teacher-rated competencies and that gains over time in family support were predictive of improved behavioral adjustment in school (Taylor & Machida, 1994; Dubow, Tisak, Causey, Hyroshko, & Reid, 1991). Reynolds’ (1989; Taylor & Machida, 1994) also concluded that teacher-rated parental involvement in school was predictive of early academic achievement and social maturity in low-income and at-risk 16 children. Overall, it was established that parent involvement has a significant impact on children’s social and emotional development. Teacher Influences. Teacher influences are described as affecting children’s social competence in a variety of ways. The strategies that teachers use to instruct, model social behaviors, plan, and discipline, all help either to enhance or restrain children’s developing attitudes and behaviors associated with social competence (Kostelnik et al., 2002). For example, in classrooms described as having a positive and child-centered social atmosphere, children displayed increased problem-solving skills, more cooperation, and less stress in comparison to children in strict, teacher-directed classrooms (Kostelnik, et al. 2002; Burts etal., 1992; Dunn & Kontos, 1997). Children in child-centered classrooms also tended to have more positive teacher relationships. Children who are able to establish these positive teacher-child relationships are noted as being more sociable, engaging in more complex play, and behaving more flexibly than children who have negative or detached relationships with their teachers (Kostelnik, et al. 2002; Elicker & Fortner-Wood, 1997). There are certain characteristics associated with teachers and their relationships with children that have been linked to the development of the child outcomes stated above. Some of these characteristics include the sensitivity of teacher’s interactions; dimensions of warmth/security/, anger/dependence, and anxiety/security; and dimensions of closeness, dependency, and conflict (Birch & Ladd, 1997). A study conducted by Birch and Ladd (1997) determined the levels of significance that existed between closeness, dependency, and conflict, in teacher-child relationships. Closeness, which encompassed a degree of warmth and open communication between the teacher and 17 child, was found to significantly support children in the school environment. Teacher’s warmth was associated with children’s positive behaviors and attitudes towards school, and open communication with the classroom teacher encouraged children to become involved and engaged in school. The degree of dependency or clinginess that children displayed towards the teacher, contrasted with closeness, and resulted in the children being less tentative in their explorations of their school environment and in their social interactions. Last, conflictual teacher-child relationships, which was characterized by discordant interactions and a lack of rapport between the teacher and the child, was found to foster feelings of anger and anxiety in children, often leading children to feelings of alienation, loneliness, and negative attitudes toward school. (Birch & Ladd, 1997) - Temperament. In addition to the biological influences mentioned above, research also finds genetic influences to be contributing factors in the development of children’s social competence. Temperament, which is often described as the “core” of one’s personality, is an inborn set of brain systems that regulates emotional intensity, reactivity, and attentional focus (Evangelista, 2002; Teglasi, 1997; and Rothbart & Jones, 1998). In general, this term is used to describe differences in children or in their behavioral styles (Landy, 2002; Chess & Thomas, 1996). According to Thomas and Chess (1977) there are three major types of temperamental styles associated with children. (1) The difficult child is described as having irregular and unpredictable routines; intense reactions; and often displays a lot of negative moods. (2) The easy child has a regular and predictable routine; readily approaches new things; shows a mild degree of reaction; and exhibits positive moods. (3) The slow-to-warm-up or shy child usually withdraws in new situations; has a low activity level; and also displays a lot of negative moods. It has been 18 found that once children display one of these styles, the characteristics associated with them usually remain stable over time. With this in mind, Thomas and Chess (1977) have coiled the term, “goodness of fit.” This term is used to emphasize the importance of the relationship between the child’s temperament and elements of his or her environment. Because friction can arise between a child’s temperamental style and the values and personality of his or her parent or caregiver, it is important that a “good fit” exists. In fact, it is the “goodness of fit” that exists between the child’s temperament and his or her environment that is an important determinant in the child’s developmental outcomes (Landy, 2002). Goodness of F it. The “goodness of fit” model examines the relationship between the individual characteristics of the child, such as capacities and styles of behavior with the expectations and demands of the environment. It is the congruence between these expectations and child characteristics that determine the fit of the child to the environment. This fit is ultimately associated with the development of social competence (De Schipper, Tavecchio, Van Ijzendoorn, & Van Zeijl, 2004). In terms of teacher-child relationships, the “goodness of fit” is determined by how well the teacher is able to adapt his or her expectations and teaching styles to fit the needs and temperament of the child. In the APEEC used in this study, this was addressed in question 13, which addressed if teacher’s expectations of children’s behaviors were appropriate for their age and abilities. Research has found that accommodating the child’s needs usually results in positive adaptive outcomes (Churchill, 2003). In a study focusing on middle schoolers’ temperaments and their teachers’ expectations in the classroom, researchers found that the fit between the child and the teacher related to 19 teacher ratings of competence, as well as standardized tests of achievement (Churchill, 2003; Lerner & Lerner, 1983; Lerner, Lerner, & Zabski, 1985; Talwar, Nitz, & Lerner, 1990). In another study examining the goodness of fit between Head Start teachers and preschool children, results concluded that the “fit” significantly related to children’s social development. More importantly, both child cognition and social outcomes were found to be the result of the “goodness of fit” between teacher and child (Churchill, 2003) Individual Influences. In addition to children’s temperamental styles, there are other individual characteristics that influence children’s social development. Two influences include having developmental delays or disorders and the child’s ability to communicate. For example, children who are identified as having developmental delays require intervention services to help them function within the domains of cognition, communication, physical development, psychosocial development, or adaptive or self- care. Children with developmental delays in one of the above domains often lag behind the norms associated with their chronological age. Although there are many reasons that children may have developmental delays, including health factors (i.e. complications resulting from a premature birth), mental retardation, and abuse (i.e. physical), studies have reported that 75 percent of these children have lower social competences or social status, regardless of the cause of their delay (Waterman, 2002; Moisan, 1998). Reviewed research by Swanson (1996) and Swanson & Malone (1992) found children with learning disabilities to be more socially rejected than their non—disabled peers. These children also engaged in or were recipients of more hostile interactions. Children with learning problems showed weaker empathy, had poor conflict resolutions skills, frequently 20 misinterpreted nonverbal messages, and were less tactful (Waterman, 2002; Swanson & Malone, 1992). Ultimately, students with learning disabilities, in comparison to students without disabilities, were found to engage in fewer social activities, were less skilled problem solvers, and showed lower levels of social competence. In terms of communication, the use of language is thought to be the root of social development (Goody, 1997). According to Goody (1997), effective language is essential in the development of social intelligence. Any problems in language processing are expected to interfere with one’s social performance. Research relating to language development found that about 50-80 percent of children with language-based disabilities exhibited poorer social, personal, and behavioral adjustments (Waterman, 2002; Bender & Smith, 1990; Bryan et al., 1998; Crick & Dodge, 1994; Cummings, Vallance, & Brazil, 1992; Schacter, Pless, & Bruck, 1991; Vallance, Cummings, & Humphries, 1998). More specifically, research findings suggest that impairments in the ability to effectively communicate thoughts and needs, along with the tendency to misinterpret the messages of others, significantly contributes to the development of poor social skills and problem behaviors (Waterman, 2002; Vallance, Cummings, & Humphries, 1998). Demographic diflerences. Although there are numerous influencing factors that contribute to children’s development of social competence, there are two predetermined elements that are noted to have a significant effect on children’s social development. These elements consist of children’s ethnicity and gender. In a study of teacher and parent assessments of social competence in inner-city children, Cartledge, Adedapo, & Johnson (1998), found that teachers consistently perceived young female students to have more social skills, and fewer behavior problems than male students. This is not 21 unexpected because these findings were consistent with the findings of other studies reporting females as being more socially competent than males (Cartledge, Adedapo, & Johnson, 1998; Gresham & Elliott, 1990; Walker, 1983). Most research in this area repeatedly shows males to be more physically aggressive and combative, in comparison to females (Cartledge, Adedapo, & Johnson, 1998; Cartledge & Milburn, 1996). In fact, the only social skill in which teacher’s perceptions did not show a significant gender difference was assertion, which includes behaviors such as initiating interactions with others and dealing with issues of being treated fairly (Cartledge, Adedapo, & Johnson, 1998) In relation to ethnicity, there is little research available on the effects that race has on the assessment of children’s behaviors. A study by Feng and Cartledge (1996) found that although there were no perceived differences between African American and Caucasian children in social skills, teachers did rate African American children as exhibiting more externalizing behaviors than their non-Black peers. Keller (1988) found that teachers also gave lower adjustment ratings to African American students. Furthermore, it has been documented that African American males are disproportionately singled out in school for disciplinary action by educators and are more likely to receive more punishing consequences and pathological labels than Caucasians (Council for Exceptional Children, 1994). With this aside, researchers attribute African American’s urban environment as having a significant influence on their social performance. As a solution, they dictate that the most salient social needs for African American males are alternatives to aggression, like more effective conflict-management skills (Cartledge, 22 Adedapo, & Johnson, 1998; Cartledge & Milburn, 1995; Hammond & Yung, 1993; Prothrow-Stith, 1991). Developmentally Appropriate Practice (DAP) The National Association for the Education of Young Children has done an enormous amount of research on the developmental needs of young children. Their policy statement for educating children from birth through age eight emphasizes the interrelationship of the physical, emotional, social, and cognitive domains of development (Bredekamp & Copple, 1997). In order to provide the best educational setting for young children, teachers are encouraged to abide by the following program guidelines directing them to: (a) recognize variations in development among children, (b) provide opportunities for active learning by allowing children to choose and plan their own activities, and (c) build social relationships between children through small group activities (Evangelista, 2002). These guidelines are designed to educate the whole child. Following these guidelines ensures that each child is acknowledged as an individual based on his or her physical, social/emotional, and intellectual growth and development, and that methods for academic learning are planned accordingly. Furthermore, DAP supports child-initiated and hands on activities, with teacher-directed instruction primarily occurring in response to an individual child’s needs. Most teacher-directed instructions should occur in short interactions between the teacher and individual child or in very brief interactive whole group activities (Moyer, 2004). DAP discourages teaching methods that are entirely whole group or which relies on pencil and paper seatwork as opposed to hands-on, active learning. 23 The Need For DAP. Due to a variety of reasons such as societal pressure, misunderstandings about how children learn, aggressive marketing of commercial materials, shortages of teachers specifically prepared to work with young children, and the reassignment of trained teachers in areas of declining enrollment, many kindergarten classrooms are now making a change for the worse (Moyer, 2004). According to present research, more kindergartens are adopting commercially prepared curricula, which are often extensions of the textbook series currently being used in early elementary grades (Stipek, Feiler, Daniels, & Milburn, 1995; Durkin, 1987; Educational Research Service, 1986; Shepard & Smith, 1988). Studies show more kindergarten classes are reporting to be whole-class or teacher-directed. Generally, this means teachers are using more formal reading instruction, written assignments, and frequent grading as a means of teaching and assessing their students (Stipek, Feiler, Daniels, & Milburn, 1995; Bryant, Clifford, & Peisner, 1991; Durkin, 1987; Hiebert, 1988; Spodek, 1982). Adoptions such as these are often thought to ensure the continuity of learning though elementary school. Instead, this change of curriculum is academically pushing our children further than they may developmentally be ready to go. As a result, pushing children into academic areas too soon has a negative affect on learning (Elkind 1996; Moyer, 2004). Many refer to this practice as the “miseducation” of young children (Moyer, 2004). Despite societal changes and what research shows, “kindergarten remains a place where children need a quality program in order to achieve their full potential” (Moyer, 2004, p.82). To reflect this, the kindergarten program should be of high quality and serve as a strong foundation upon which children build skills, knowledge, and attitudes towards school and lifelong learning. Based on this information, making a positive change in the 24 kindergarten environment is a major issue that needs to be addressed. In relation to the standards of the NAEYC and The Association for Childhood Education International, changes should be made based on three major areas: physical environment, instructional context, and social context (Bredekamp & Copple, 1997; 1987). Components of DAP Physical Environment. According to Bronfenbrenner (197 9, p.845), a child’s development is enhanced in an environment where children can observe and participate in “ongoing patterns of progressively more complex activity” and are guided by persons “with which the child had developed a positive emotional relationship.” This environment should make the children feel safe and secure. To ensure that optimal- learning occurs, children should have, “opportunity, resources, and encouragement to engage in the activities” on their own (Bronfenbrenner, 1979, p.845). Joan Moyer (2004, p. 82) comments, “kindergarten children are active, curious learners who need adequate space, a variety of materials, and large blocks of times in which to try out their ideas.” To facilitate learning, teachers should arrange their classroom based on the children’s interest, level of interaction and involvement, developmental stage, skill development, and overall attitude towards school and learning (Moyer, 2004). The physical environment of the room should be set up to accommodate various teaching styles (i.e. large group, small group, and individual learning activities), facilitate movement, and display learning materials. To provide children with the best opportunity for learning, these materials should be changed and combined to increase levels of complexity to help children become more self-directed and involved. 25 Instructional Context. Educators and developmental psychologist have debated for many years over the effects that different instructional approaches have on young children’s learning and social development. As discussed earlier, many kindergarten classrooms are currently conforming to the teaching styles of elementary classrooms, which use teacher-directed activities, textbooks, and worksheets to educate young children. Based on NAEYC’s national standards, this form of teaching is inappropriate and unacceptable. Instead of using whole group methods, kindergarten teachers are encouraged to provide children with a variety of activities and materials to promote and encourage “hands-on” activities, communication, and problem-solving skills (Bredekamp & Copple, 1997). Children should be given an opportunity to work in various settings, consisting of whole-group instruction (i.e. circle time), small groups, and chances to work independently at learning centers suited to the children’s wide range of skills, interests, and needs. Recent studies focusing on the social-emotional effects of didactic teaching environments compared to child-initiated or DAP environments found that preschool children enrolled in the child-initiated programs display lower levels of test anxiety than children enrolled in the academic program (Hyson, Hirsh-Pasek, & Rescorla, 1990). Children in inappropriate, academically based programs exhibited more total stress behaviors through the day and more stress behaviors during group times and workbook/worksheet activities than children in child-initiated programs (Burts, Hart, Charlesworth, & Kirk, 1990). In terms of cognition or academic achievement, children in child-initiated programs scored higher on measures of creativity, or divergent thinking, in comparison to 26 children in academically based programs (Dunn & Konots, 1997; Hyson, Hirsh-Pasek, & Rescorla, 1990). Children in DAP classrooms also had better outcomes in language development. They were reported to have better verbal and receptive language skills than children in academically based programs (Dunn & Kontos, 1997; Marcon, 1992). Social Context. Finally, early childhood teachers need to understand the influence of social cultural contexts on learning, recognizing children’s developing competence, and accepting a variety of ways for children to express their developmental achievements. As a part of the social context of the classroom, teachers should learn about the cultures of all of the children in their classroom and use this diversity as a guideline for teaching, being sure to encompass all ideas and backgrounds into their classroom. Encouraging this type of social interaction plays a significant role in the cognitive development of young children. According to the developmental standards of NAEYC, children’s social context should enable them to have an active role in the decision-making process in their classroom (Bredekamp & Copple, 1997). A positive adult-child relationship should exist for children to feel safe and secure. Once these feelings are established, children can look to teachers to provide instruction, model social behaviors and attitudes, plan the physical environment, formulate routines, and provide discipline and guidance (Kostelnik et al., 2002). Outcomes Associated With DAP. Current research shows that children from families with low socioeconomic status (SES) begin school, on average, with significantly poorer academic skills than more advantaged children (Stipek & Rosaleen, 1997; Bryant, Burchinal, Lau, & Sparling, 1994; Goldenberg, Reese, & Gallirnore, 1992; Griffin, Case, & Siegler, 1994; Jordan, Huttenlocher, & Levine, 1992; Starkey & Klein, 27 1992). In reality, these children are considered to be extremely behind and are forced to begin school trying to “catch- up.” In fact, most of the preschool environments these disadvantaged children attended were developmentally inappropriate, which often led to negative outcomes, including relatively greater declines in school achievement by the fourth grade and poor social adjustment in adolescence (Huffman & Speer, 2000; Marcon, 1995; Schweinhart, Weikart & Lamer, 1986). Without assistance or a proper educational environment, these children are being setup to fail. On the other hand, DAP programs have been found to lead to more positive outcomes in young children. In a DAP environment, the teachers plans the curriculum with the children’s interest in mind. They provide engaging learning experiences in which the children can succeed, while also being challenged to work on the edge of their developing capabilities (Bredekamp & Copple, 1997). Supporting studies of DAP have found positive outcomes in children in relation to academic, social, and behavioral domains (Hoffinan & Speer, 2000). By the first grade most students who were enrolled in developmentally appropriate classrooms had significantly higher cognitive skills, in comparison to students in less developmentally appropriate classrooms. The positive long term effects of DAP are indicated by higher rates of high school graduation, fewer arrests and acts of misconduct, and higher monthly incomes as adults (Schweinhart & Weikart, 1993; Huffman & Speer, 2000). More specifically, most research indicates that more DAP has a positive effect on academic and behavioral outcomes and mitigate stress related behaviors of lower socioeconomic, nonmainstream, non-European American, and male students (Hart et al. 1998; Hart et al. 1997). 28 Summary Many studies have been conducted to investigate how developmentally appropriate practices influence children’s cognitive and emotional development. In terms of the classroom environment, teachers have been found to have a major impact on children’s social development. The strategies they use to instruct, model social behaviors, plan, and discipline, all help to enhance or restrain children’s developing attitudes and behaviors associated with social competence (Kostelnik et al., 2002). Current research examining teacher behaviors in DAP programs, have reported that children develop more positive attitudes and have higher success rates in school (Schweinhart & Weikart, 1993; Huffman & Speer, 2002). With this in mind, the results in Chapter IV will present information relevant to the relationship that exists between children’s social competency skills and components of the developmental appropriateness of their classroom environment as measured by the APEEC. 29 CHAPTER III METHODS Research Design This was a correlational study involving an analysis of kindergarten children’s social competency skills and the developmental appropriateness of their classroom environment. The subjects of this study were enrolled in one of eleven different kindergarten classrooms selected from eight elementary schools located in an urban, Midwest school district. Analysis of secondary data collected from a larger study was used for this investigation. Participants The total number of participants for this study included 11 female kindergarten teachers and 166 kindergarten children, of which 88 were males and 78 were females. The mean age of the children during the initial testing was approximately five years and 6 months old (Mean= 66.80 months, SD= 5.36). Table 1 presents the age of the kindergarten children during the initial testing in November. Table 1. Gender and Age of Children During Initial Testing in November Variable N Mean (SD.) Male 86 66.80 (5.46) Female 77 66.79 (5.27) Total 163 66.80 (5.36) This study also utilized demographic variables including the race/ethnicity of the children. The ethnic composition of the sample, listed in Table 2, illustrates that the 30 largest group of the children participating in this study were Afiican-American (N =75, 45.2%). Table 2. Ethnicity of Children Variable Frequency % Ethnicity African American 75 45.2 Caucasian 51 30.7 Hispanic 30 l 8.1 Native American 5 3.0 Asian 3 1.8 Other 2 1.2 Total 166 100 Research Questions The research questions addressed in this study are listed on page 10 in Chapter One. Measures Two instruments were utilized in this study to assess the social competence of kindergarten children and the developmental appropriateness of their classroom environment at the end of kindergarten. These instruments included the Pre- K/Kindergarten Social Competency Scale (PK/K-S) (Soderman, 2002), and the Assessment of Practices in Early Elementary Classrooms (APEEC) (Hemmeter et al., 2001). A copy of the PK/K-S instrument is included in Appendix A. Pre-K/Kindergarten Social Competency Scale (PK/K-S). The teachers in this study were requested to complete the PK/K-S (Soderman, 2002) twice during the year, to measure the kindergarteners’ social competency skills. This rating scale was produced 31 specifically for the teachers involved in this program by combining readiness indicators from the Head Start Checklist, children’s report cards, and social competency skills as outlined in Developmentally Appropriate Curriculum (Kostelnik, Soderman, & Whiren, 2004). Because this readiness scale is yet to be published, Soderman (personal communication, 2004) verifies that this instrument has content validity because the selected variables included in the assessment scale have been reviewed and edited by other experts in the fields of early childhood education and child development. Because the internal reliability of the instrument was not determined, a reliability analysis was conducted on this scale (see Chapter V). The intended purpose of this instrument was to provide kindergarten and Head Start teachers with a measurement that would document the occurrence of specific skills as they relate to social competency (i.e. comforts or helps others who are upset, hurt, or having difficulty with a task). This instrument is comprised of twenty-five items. Teachers were expected to rate each item on a four-point frequency scale (4= often characteristic, 3= sometimes characteristic, 2 = rarely characteristic, and 1= not characteristic). Teacher ratings were done twice during the school year, in the months of November and May. These scores were used to determine if a relationship existed between children’s social competency scores and teachers’ scores on the APEEC. The difference in the scores of the first and last ratings were computed to show if children’s social competency scores changed over time. Assessment of Practices in Early Elementary Classrooms (APEEC). The classroom attended by the kindergarten children in this study was assessed for its 32 developmental appropriateness using the Assessment of Practices in Early Elementary Classrooms (APEEC) (Hemmeter, et al., 2001). This instrument is divided into three broad domains of classroom practices: physical environment, instructional context, and social context. The APEEC consists of forty items, scored along a 7-point continuum of developmentally appropriate classroom practices, with descriptors at the “1”, “3”, “5”, and “7” anchors (i.e. “’1’ indicates the classroom is inadequate in terms of developmentally appropriate practices, ‘3’ indicates minimal developmentally appropriate practices, ‘5’ indicates that classroom is good in terms of developmental appropriateness, and ‘7’ indicates excellent developmentally appropriate practices”) (Hemmeter, et al., 2001). To obtain the classroom score, the total sum of the sixteen item-level scores used to measure the physical environment, instructional context, and social context was divided by the total number of items scored. Higher scores on the APEEC reflect higher quality classrooms and according to the authors, should be associated with positive child outcomes (Hemmeter, et al., 2001). Interrater agreement and validity data for the APEEC were established using 38 K-3 classrooms in the spring of 1997. Results showed that the APEEC was an internally consistent, valid measure of developmentally appropriate practices. However, the interrater agreement data was low. Based on those results, the authors conducted a series of revisions and pilot tests in 1998. As a result, the data from the revised field-tests suggested that the APEEC was an internally consistent, valid measure of developmentally appropriate practices that had good interrater agreement (Hemmeter, et al., 2001 ). Interrater agreement and validity data were also collected using 69 classrooms, grades K-3, in North Carolina and Kentucky. Overall, the interrater agreement was 33 reported to be high at the descriptor level, item level, and total score level. At the descriptor level, the percentage of agreement among two observers averaged 86%, ranging from 76% to 93% across 135 descriptors. The exact average of the item level percentage of agreement was 58%, ranging from 31% to 81%. The Kappa statistic was also calculated to determine the measure of reliability that accounts for chance agreement and the degree of disagreement between observers. The Kappa statistic was generally weighted .50 or higher for 12 items of the sixteen item level scores; however, 2 of the 4 remaining items fell below .47. The median weighted Kappa was .59. The interrater agreement of this measure between two observer ratings was .86, which suggested that a high level of interrater agreement was established with the APEEC. Construct validity was also determined by comparing the APEEC with two other measures, the Assessment Profile for Early Childhood Programs (Abbott-Shim, & Sibley, 1988) and the Teachers Beliefs and Practices Scale (TBPS) (Buchanan, Burts, Bidner, White, & Charlesworth, 1998; Charlesworth, Hart, Burts, Thomassom, Mosley, & Fleege, 1993; Hemmeter et al., 2001). The modest-to-high-correlations between the measures suggest that the APEEC is a valid measure of developmentally appropriate practices. Data Collection Procedure The data used in this study were previously collected during the 2002-2003 school year for the Leap Frog Program (Soderman & Farrell, 2003). This study utilized a part of the database that included the children’s level of social competence and developmental ' appropriateness of their teacher’s classrooms. To assess the children’s level of social competence, classroom teachers were asked to complete the PK/K-S assessment on each child in their classroom twice during 34 the school year. The difference between the November and May rating were computed to show if children’s social competency scores changed over time. This study also used the November and May scores obtained on the PK/K-S to determine if a relationship existed between children’s social competency scores and their teachers’ scores received on the APEEC. Each elementary classroom was assessed for approximately one hour by two observers, using the APEEC. Observers were also privy to children’s social competence, though formal assessments were not done. A fifteen-minute teacher interview was also conducted to obtain additional information listed at the end of each section on the APEEC. After completing each assessment, the observers met to discuss their observations before coming to a mutual consensus about the score assigned in each category. In each case, there was high interrater reliability between the scores given on the APEEC. However, interrater reliability was not statistically determined. Data Analysis Relationships between the variables in this study were determined using a multiple regression. These variables included kindergarteners’ November and May social competency scores from the PK/K-S, and the sub scores and total score of the APEEC used to assess the developmental appropriateness of the classroom environment. In order to investigate how social competency scores relate to the subscales of the APEEC and to determine if teachers with higher APEEC scores have children with higher social competency scores, a multiple regression model was created. To determine if demographic variables contributed to the differences in children’s PK/K-S scores given by their teachers, a T-Test and a One-Way ANOVA was conducted. 35 A T-Test was run to indicate if children’s scores varied by gender, and a One-Way ANVOA was conducted to indicate if PK/K-S scores varied according to ethnicity. Additionally, a paired sample t-test was run to compare scores of the November PK/K-S and the May PK/K-S, to determine if there was any change in children’s behavior over the course of the school year. Alpha was set at .05. A reliability analysis was also conducted on the PK/K-S measure of social competency to determine the instrument’s level of internal consistency. Limitations 1. Due to the sample size and location in which this study took place, the results cannot be generalized to a population larger than its assessable population, which includes all kindergarten children in an urban, Midwest school district. Teachers were solely used to report on their students’ social competency skills, which could have resulted in biased behavioral ratings. It would have been useful to have another source of data on children’s social competence. . The PK/K-S had not been tested for its degree of validity and reliability. Moreover, if it was not used with integrity by the classroom teacher, it would not be a reliable measure of children’s social competence. Because the observers did not complete independent score sheets for the APEEC classroom assessment, interrater reliability was not established. No demographic information was collected on the children’s families. For example, information relating to ethnicity, age, education level of parent(s), income, family type, and number of family members living in the same household were not obtained. 36 6. No demographic information was collected on teachers. As a result, information relating to their age, ethnicity, educational background, early childhood training, and number of years spent teaching kindergartner could not be included. 7. Demographics collected on the children’s previous educational background were not specific and only included information about their enrollment in Head Start and Michigan School Readiness Programs. Additional information on their involvement in preschool or other childcare services was not collected. Therefore, this information could not be utilized. 8. No information was collected on individual or peer influences, as mentioned in Chapters I and II, to be contributing factors to the development of children’s social competency. 37 CHAPTER IV RESULTS Results All data for this study were collected from eleven kindergarten classrooms in an urban, Midwest school district. The data for analyzing children’s social competence were measured using the PK/K-S. The developmental appropriateness of the classroom environment was measured using the APEEC. These two variables were analyzed to investigate the relationship between children’s social competence and the developmental appropriateness of their classroom environment. The findings for the PK/K-S’s internal consistency and for each research question are presented as followed. Reliability-internal consistency A reliability analysis conducted on the 25- item PK/K-S showed the Cronbach’s alpha for the November and May assessment, possessed a high level of internal consistency and reliability. These results are listed below in Table 3. Table 3. Reliability Analysis Scale Mean Cronbach’s Alpha November PK/K-S 3.18 .95 (N =160) May PK/K-S 3.51 .91 (N=163) 38 Variance in gender and ethnicity in children’s social competency score. To determine if children’s social competency scores varied by gender, an independent t-test was run. The results are listed below in Table 4. A One-Way ANOVA was also conducted to determine if children’s scores varied by ethnicity. The results for this analysis are presented in Table 5. Research Question 1: Do teacher ratings of children’s social competence vary depending on demographic variables, such as gender and ethnicity? An independent t-test determined that by gender, there was a significant relationship in children’s scores on the PK/K-S in November and in May (t= -4.11-, p=.000). Female scores were higher in November and May, than the scores of their male classmates. In relation to ethnicity, a One-Way AN OVA determined that there were no significant ethnic differences in children’s social competency scores in November or in May. Table 4. T-Test for differences between males and females in November and May Group N Mean SD t Prob. Male 86 75.59 13.57 -4.11 .000* Female 76 84.00 12.34 (In November) Male 87 84.89 14.35 -2.45 .015 Female 78 89.87 11.48 (In May) 39 Table 5. Results of a One-Way ANOVA analysis of variance for ethnicity in children’s PK/K-S scores Groups Sum of Squares df Mean Squares f-value Prob. Between 539.44 4 134.85 .712 .585 Within 293 78.41 155 189.54 Total 29917.84 159 (In November) Between 61.36 4 15.34 .085 .987 Within 28419.82 158 179.87 Total 28481.18 162 (In May) Relationship between social competency scores over the course of the school year. In regards to research question 2, a paired t-test was conducted to evaluate differences between mean scores on the PK/K-S over the course of the school year. The results in Table 6 compare the significant differences in the mean scores the kindergarten children received on the PK/K-S in November and in May. Research Question 2: Do children’s social competency skills change over the course of the school year? The children’s scores in May with a mean of 86.14 (SD 13.40), were higher than their November scores, which had a mean of 79.74 (SD 13.43). This was a difference of 6.25 between the two assessments. With an alpha level of .05, the difference in scores were found to be statistically significant, (t =-7.97, p= .000). 40 Table 6. Paired T-Test: Social Competency Scores in November and May (161) Test Time Mean SD Std. Error Mean T-Test Level of Sig. November PK/K-S 79.74 (13.43) 1.06 .000* May PK/K-S 87.14 (13.40) 1.06 .000* Relationships between kindergarten children ’s social competency scores and variables of developmentally appropriate classrooms. A multiple regression was created to determine the relationship that existed between the November and May assessments of the PK/K-S and the three subscales and total score of the APEEC. Tables 7-10 illustrate the results of research questions 3-6. Research Question 3: How do kindergarten children’s social competency abilities, rated in November and May, relate to their teacher’s physical environment scores obtained on the Assessment of Practices in Early Elementary Classrooms (APEEC)? In regards to the teacher’s physical environment scores obtained on the APEEC, a significant relationship was found (F=6.6, p<.05) between the November ratings children received on the PK/K-S and their teacher’s physical environment scores obtained on the APEEC. However, there were no significant relationships between children’s May ratings and their teacher’s physical environment scores on the APEEC. 41 Table 7. Multiple Regression Analysis: Physical environment as a predictor of kindergarten children’s social competence. Outcome: November and May scores on the PK/K-S Predictor variables b SE b t-Statistic Prob. R sq F -Ratio Physical environment 4.19 2.08 2.01 <.001* .11 6.58 (November) Research Question 4: How do kindergarten children’s social competency abilities, rated in November and May, relate to their teacher’s instructional context scores obtained on the APEEC? There is a significant relationship between children’s social competency scores and their teacher’s instructional scores obtained on the APEEC in November (F=6.6, p<.05). An increase in teachers’ instructional scores is positively related to increases in children’s social competency scores. As for children’s social competency scores in May, no significant relationships were found. Table 8. Multiple Regression Analysis: Instructional context as a predictor of kindergarten children’s social competence. Outcome: November and May scores on the PK/K-S Predictor variables b SE b t-Statistic Prob. R sq F-Ratio Physical environment 4.11 1.27 3.23 <.001* .1 1 6.58 (November) 42 Research Question 5: How do kindergarten children’s social competency abilities, rated in November and May, relate to their teacher’s social context scores obtained on the APEEC? A significant relationship between children’s social competency scores and their teacher’s social context scores were found in November (F=6.6, p<.05). An increase in teachers’ social context scores is positively related to increases in children’s social competency scores. However, no significant relationships were found in May. Table 9. Multiple Regression Analysis: Social context as a predictor of kindergarten children’s social competence. Outcome: November and May scores on the PK/K-S Predictor variables b SE b t-Statistic Prob. R sq F -Ratio Social context -3.91 1.84 -2.12 <.001* .11 6.58 (November) p<.05* Research Question 6: How do kindergarten children’s social competency abilities, rated in November and May, relate to their teacher’s total score obtained on the APEEC? A significant relationship exists between children’s social competency scores and the total score teachers received on the APEEC in November (F=9.7, p<.05). An increase in teachers’ total scores is positively related to increases in children’s social competency scores. However, in May, no significant relationships between the variables were found. 43 Table 10. Multiple Regression Analysis: Total scores on the APEEC as a predictor of kindergarten children’s social competence. Outcome: November and May scores on the PK/K-S Predictor variables b SE b t-Statistic Prob. R sq. F -Ratio Total score 3.33 1.07 3.11 .003* .06 9.69 (November) Summary of Data This study was designed to investigate the relationships between children’s social competence and the developmental appropriateness of their classroom environment. A reliability analysis on the PK/K-S concluded that the instrument had a high level of internal consistency based on Chronbach’s alpha. An independent t-test determined there was a significant relationship in the November and May PK/K-S scores by gender. Additionally, a One-Way AN OVA found no significant difference in the November and May PK/K-S scores between ethnic groups. This study did, however, show there was a significant positive change in children’s social competency scores over the course of the school year. Results of research questions 3-6 indicate that all three components of developmental appropriateness, as well as the total scores teachers obtained on the APEEC were significantly related to children’s social competency scores measured in November. On the contrary, results showed there were no significant relationships between children’s social competency scores and the developmental appropriateness of their classroom environment, as measured by the APEEC in May. Furthermore, when rated 44 according to subscales, no significant relationships between kindergarten children’s level of social competence and the developmental appropriateness of their teacher’s physical environment, instructional context, or social context were found. A table listing the significance of these findings is presented below (see Table 11). 45 Table 11. Summary of Findings Research Question Findings 1. Do teacher ratings of children’s social competence vary depending on demographic variables, such as gender and ethnicity? 2. Do children’s social competency skills change over the course of the school year? 3. How do kindergarten children’s social competency abilities, rated in November and May, relate to their teacher’s physical context scores measured on the APEEC? A t of —4.43 (p<.001) was found in November, and t of —2.89 (p<.001)was found in May for variation in gender differences for teacher ratings. No differences were found among ethnic groups in social competence scores. With a t of —4.24 (p<.001) children’s social competency scores were shown to increase from November to May with a difference of 6.25 between the two assessments. An F of 6.6 (p<.05), found a significant relationship to exist in November. No significant relationship was found in May. 46 4. How do kindergarten children’s social competency abilities, rated in November and May, relate to their teacher’s instructional context scores measured on the APEEC? How do kindergarten children’s social competency abilities, rated in November and May, relate to their teacher’s social context scores measured on the APEEC? How do kindergarten children’s social competency abilities, rated in November and May, relate to their teacher’s total scores obtained on the APEEC? An F of 6.6 (p<.05) determined a significant relationship to exist in November. No significant relationship was found in May. An F of 6.6 (p<.05) determined a significant relationship to exist in November. No significant relationship was found in May. An F of 9.7 (p<.05) determined a significant to exist in November. No significant relationship was found in May. 47 CHAPTER V DISCUSSION Discussion The purpose of this study was to investigate the relationship between teachers’ developmentally appropriate practices observed in kindergarten classrooms and children’s social competency skills. Specifically, developmentally appropriate practices were rated according to three subscales: physical environment, instructional context, and social context, using the APEEC. Children’s social competence was measured using the PK/K-S. A regression analysis revealed a strong and significant relationship between children’s social competency scores measured in November and teachers’ scores measured on the APEEC. This indicated that in November, teachers’ classroom practices, particularly their method of instruction, were related to the development of children’s social competence. On the other hand, there were no significant relationships between children’s social competency scores measured in May and their teachers’ scores measured on the APEEC. It is believed that the teachers’ integrity when completing the PK/K-S may have also contributed to the non-significant findings of the May assessments. According to a study examining teacher assessments of social competence, “teacher ratings have been shown to be good initial evaluations of children’s social behaviors” (Cartledge, Adedapo, & Johnson, 1998, p. 116; Greenwood, Walker, Todd, & Hops, 1979; Walker, et al., 198 8). Assuming this to be true, and based on observations of the children and teachers within their classroom environment, it is believed that the ratings teachers completed in 48 November were valid and reliable. However, based on the significantly increased scores some children received and the way in which some teachers completed the May assessment of the PK/K-S, the researcher was led to believe that the May assessments were not a valid or reliable assessment of children’s social competence. Tables 12 shows an example of how one teacher rated a student who was scored as having a low level of social competence in November. Table 13 illustrates the exceptional increase in the score the child received during the May assessment. These tables also show the difference in the teacher’s scoring technique. In November, the teacher scored each question separately; however, in May, the teacher used a squiggly line to indicate that the child exhibited all of these behaviors often. This teacher was reported as completing twelve out of thirteen student PK/K-S assessments in this manner. Observations of this teacher’s classroom by several of the researchers deemed her May assessment as invalid and without integrity. 49 Table 12. PK/K-S measured in November. —— Directions: Rate characteristic of child from 1-4, 1=not characteristic; 2=rare1y characteristic; 3=sometimes characteristic; 4=often characteristic. 10. 11. 12. 13. 14. 15. 16. 17. 18. 19. 20. . Social Competency Skills . Separates from parent without distress Verbalizes feelings . Asks for help when necessary . Works and plays cooperatively with others . When angry, knows how to calm self down . Puts away materials where they belong; respects materials and equipment . Respects adults . Waits turn before speaking . Follows the rules of the classroom and school without reminders Helps to clean up after activities Can usually resolve own conflicts with others Is friendly and helpful to others Is liked by other children; attracts friends Capable of making good learning choices without adult intervention Follows directions well Adjusts to changes in routine without undue stress Comforts or helps others who are upset. hurt, or having difficulty with a task Can wait his/her tum Sticks to an activity for as long as can be expected for a child this age Shares materials, equipment with others 50 Isle Isl- l9 t—lrlrlrl'lvlrlt’l‘lw Time 1 Time2 Time 3 21. Acce ts new activities without fear or uctanee 22. Has an adequate amount of energy during the class period 23. Eager to participate in classroom activities 24. Shows self confidence on Moat» lbs 25. Stays on task when necessary; attemfi' to finish a task Total Number of Skills Identified: 56 :Zj _ __ Two to three skills to actively target prior to next testing: sums after Test 1: / FD\\ou) &\‘m'*10 as v CW Mm} u I Skills after Test 2: 51 Table 13. PK/K-S measured in May. ———. Directions: Rate characteristic of child from 1-4, l=not characteristic; 2=rarely characteristic; 3=sometimes characteristic; 4=often characteristic. O‘M‘bUN \l 10. ll. 12. 15. 16. l7. l8. 19. 20. . Social Competency Skills . Separates from parent without distress . Verbalizes feelings . Asks for help when necessary . Works and plays cooperatively with others . Puts away materials where they belong; respects materials and equipment . Respects adults . Waits turn before speaking without reminders Helps to clean up after activities . When angry, knows how to calm self down 7 . Follows the rules of the classroom and school Can usually resolve own conflicts with others Is friendly and helpful to others . ls liked by other children; attracts friends 14. Capable of making good learning choices without adult intervention' Follows directions well Adjusts to changes in routine without undue stress Comforts or helps others who are upset. hurt. or having difficulty with a task Can wait his/her turn Sticks to an activity for as long as can be expected for a child this age Shares materials, equipment with others 52 Time 1 Time2 __ Algae; —_ Time 3 21. Accepts new activities without fear or reluctance 22. Has an adequate amount of energy during the class period 23. Eager to participate in classroom activities 24. Shows self confidence 25. Stays on task when necessary; attemps to finish a task Total Number of Skills Identified: Two to three skills to actively target prior to next testing: Skills afterTest 1: li Skills after Test 2: 53 As mentioned earlier in Chapter 3, observers were privy to children’s social competence, even though formal assessments were not completed by the observers. In relation to this child, more than one observer who spent a reasonable amount of time in this classroom highly disagreed with the score this child received on the May PK/K-S. This child talked back to the teacher, refused to complete instructed tasks, and often did not follow directions (refer to questions 7, 9, 12, and 15 on Tables). Having observed these behaviors and comparing them with the teacher scores, it is highly unlikely that the teacher completed these assessments with integrity or validity. Implications for fixture research and practice It is important that future research on the relationship between developmentally appropriate practices and social competence be conducted. Based on the findings of this research study, implications for fiature studies are listed below. Because research has found teacher-child relationships, parent-child relationships, peer relationships, and temperamental styles of teachers, parents, and children to influence the development of social competence, it is proposed that these influences, in addition to the components of developmental appropriateness used in this study, be assessed and analyzed to determine which factor(s) contribute most significantly to children’s development of social competence. Also, children may manifest differing levels of competence in different contexts. Therefore, it is also strongly suggested that these measures be assessed at least twice during the school year, preferably, at the beginning and end. Each assessment should be measured at or around the same time to allow the researcher to examine the development of social competence in relation to these predictor variables. Doing this will also give researchers a more thorough understanding 54 of what factor(s) have the most significant influence on children’s social development, and at what time during the school year. As mentioned as a limitation of this study (see pages 44-45), it is also recommended that in addition to the teacher, another participant (i.e. parent or observer), who interacts with the children on a consistent basis, complete the PK/K-S assessments. This will allow researchers to examine levels of inter-rater reliabity and eliminate any biased scores that may be given by the teacher or observer alone. Finally, another opportunity for expansion upon this study would be to obtain additional demographic information on the teachers and students. Collecting information about teacher beliefs, teaching experience, age, degree or certifications, and ethnicity will allow further analysis of teachers’ influence and children’s social competence. Information about children’s SES status, prior schooling, temperament style, and disabilities should also be obtained. This information could be used to generalize if there are any specific child characteristics that relate to the development of social competence. 55 APPENDIX A PK/K Early Readiness Scale 56 Directions: Rate characteristics of child from 1-4. l= not characteristic; 2= rarely characteristic; 3= sometimes characteristic; 4= often characteristic. Time 1 Time 2 Time 3 A. Social Competency Skills Separates form parent without distress Verbalizes feelings Asks for help when necessary Works and plays cooperatively with others When angry, knows how to calm self down Puts away materials where they belong; respects materials and equipment Respects adults Waits turn before speaking 9. Follows the rules of the classroom and school without reminders 10. Helps to clean up after activities 11. Can usually resolve own conflicts with others 12. Is friendly and helpful to others 13. Is liked by other children; attracts friends 14. Capable of making good learning choices without adult intervention 15. Follows directions well 16. Adjusts to changes in routine without undue stress 17. Comforts or helps others who are upset, hurt, or having difficulty with task 18. Can wait his/her turn 19. Sticks to an activity for as long as can be expected for a child this age 20. Shares materials, equipment with others 21. Accepts new activities without fear or reluctance QMPP’NT‘ 23°.\) 22. Has an adequate amount of energy during the class period 23. Eager to participate in classroom activities 24. Show self confidence 25. Stays on task when necessary; attempts to finish task Total Number of Skills Identified: 57 REFERENCES 58 REFERENCES Abbott-Shim, M., & Sibley, A. (1988). Assessment profile for early childhood programs. Atlanta, GA: Quality Assist. Azmitia, M., Montgomery, R. (1993). Friendship, transactive dialogues, and the development of scientific reasoning. Social Development, 2, 202-221. Bates, L., Luster, T., & Vandenbelt, M. (2003). Social competence in elementary school. Social Development, 12 (1), 107-124. Baumrind, D. (1995). Child maltreatment and optimal caregiving in social contexts. New York: Garland Publishing, 1995. Bender, W. N., & Smith, J. K. (1990). Classroom behavior of children and adolescents with learning disabilities: A meta-analysis. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 23, 298-305. Bemdt, T. J. (1989). Contributions of peer relationships to children’s development. In T. J. Bemdt & G. W. Ladd (Eds), Peer relationships in child development (pp. 407- 416). New York: Wiley. Bemdt, T. J. (1996). Exploring the effects of friendship quality on social development. In W. M. Bukowski, A. F. Newcomb, & W. W. Hartup (Eds), The company they keep: Friendship in childhood and adolescence (pp. 346-365). New York: Cambridge University Press. Bemdt, T. J ., & Keefe, K (1995). Friends’ influence on adolescents’ adjustment to school. Child Development, 66 (5), 1312-1329. Birch, S. H., & Ladd, G. W. (1996, April). Continuity and change in the quality of teacher-child relationships: links with children ’s early school adjustment. Paper presented at the American Educational Research Association meetings, New York. Birch, S. H., & Ladd, G. W. (1997). The teacher-child relationship and children’s early school adjustment. Journal of School Psychology, 35 (1), 61-79. Bloch, M. N. (1992). Critical perspectives on the historical relationship between child development and early childhood education research. In S. Kessler & B. B. Swadener (Eds), Reconceptualizing the early childhood curriculum: Beginning the dialogue (pp. 3-20). New York: Teachers College Press. Bobbit, N., & Paolucci, B. (1986). Strengths of the home and family as learning environments. In Griffore, R. J ., & Boger, R. P. (Eds), Child rearing in the home and school. (pp. 47-60). New York: Plenum. 59 Bredekamp, S., & Copple, C. (1997). Developmentally appr0priate practice in early childhood programs. Washington, DC: National Association for the Education for the Education of Young Children. Bredekamp, 8., ed. 1987. Developmentally appropriate practice in early childhood programs serving children from birth through age 8. Exp. ed. Washington, DC: NAEYC. Bronfenbrenner U. (1979). The ecology of human development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Bronfenbrenner, U. (1989). “Ecological Systems Theory ” Annals of Child Development v.6, p. 189-249. Greenwich, Connecticut: JAI Press, Inc. Bronfenbrennner, U. (1993). The ecology of cognitive development research models and fugitive findings. In Wozniak, R.H., & Fischer, K.W. (Eds.), Development in Context. (pp. 3-44). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Bryan, T., Sullivan-Burstein, K., & Mather, S. (1998). The influence of affect on social- information-processing. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 3], 418-426. Bryant, D., Burchinal, M., Lau, L., & Sparling, J. (1994). Family and classroom correlates of Head Start children's developmental outcomes. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 9, 289-309. Bryant, D., Clifford, R., & Peisner, E. (1991). Best practices for beginners: Developmentally appropriateness in kindergarten. American Educational Research Journal, 28, 783-803. Buchanan, T. K., Burts, D. C., Bidner, J ., White, F ., & Charlesworth, R. (1998). Predictors of the developmentally appropriateness of beliefs and practices of first, second, and third grade teachers. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 13, 459- 483. Bukowski, W. M., Newcomb, A. F., Hartup, W. W. (1996a). Friendship and its significance in childhood and adolescence: Introduction and comment. In W. M. Bukowski, A. F. Newcomb, & W. W. Hartup, (Eds.), The company they keep: Friendship in childhood and adolescence (pp. 1-15). New York: Cambridge University Press. Bulbolz, M. & Sontag, M. (1993). Human ecology theory. In Boss, P., Doherty, W., La Rossa, R., Schumm, W., & Steinmetz, S. (Eds.), Sourcebook of family theories and methods: A contextual approach. (pp.419-448). New York: Plenum Press. Burts, D. C., Hart, C. H., Charlesworth, R., Fleege, P., Mosley, J ., & Thomasson, R. H. (1992). Observed activities and stress behaviors of children in developmentally 6O appropriate and inappropriate kindergarten classrooms. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 7, 297-318. Burts, D. C., Hart, C. H., Charlesworth, R., & Kirk, L. (1990). A comparison of frequencies of stress behaviors observed in kindergarten children in classrooms with developmentally appropriate vs. developmentally inappropriate practices. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 7, 297-318. Burts, D. C., Hart, C. H., Charlesworth, R., DeWolf, M., Ray, J ., Manuel, K., & Fleege, P. O. (1993). Developmental appropriateness of kindergarten programs and academic outcomes in first grade. Journal of Research in Childhood Education, 8 (1 ), 23-31. Carmella, G. S. (1997). Early childhood education: A call for the construction of revolutionary images. In W. F. Pinar (Ed.), Curriculum: New identities for the field (pp. 157-184). New York: Garland Publishing. Cartledge, G, & Milburn, J. F. ( 1995). Teaching social skills to children and youth Innovative approaches. Boston: Allyn & Bacon. Cartledge, G., & Milburn, J .F. (1996). Cultural diversity and social skill teachings: Understandings in ethnic and gender issues. Charnpaign, IL: Research Press. Cartledge, G., Adpapo, V., & Johnson, GT. (1998). Teacher and parent assessments of the social competence of inner-city children. The Journal of Negro Education, 67 (2), 115-126. Clarke-Stewart, A., Friedman, S., & Koch, J. (1994). Children at home and in day care. Hillsdale, NJ: Earlbaum. Charlesworth, R., Hart, C. H., Burts, D. C., & Hernandez, S. (199]). Kindergarten teachers beliefs and practices. Early Child Development and Care, 70, 17-35. Charlesworth, R., Hart, C. H., Burts, D. C., Thommason, R. H., Mosley, J., & Fleege, P. (1993). Measuring the developmental appropriateness of kindergarten teachers’ beliefs and practices. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 8 (3), 255-276. Chess, 3., & Thomas, A. (1996). Temperament: Theory and practice. New York: Brunner/Mazel. Churchill, S. L. (2003). Goodness-of-fit in early childhood settings. Early Childhood Education Journal, 31 (2), 113-118. Council for Exceptional Children. (January, 1994). Statistical profile of special education in the United States, 1994. Supplement to Teaching Exceptional Children, (26 (3), 1-4 61 Crick, N. R., & Dodge, K. A. (1994). A review and reformulation of social inforrnation- processing mechanisms in children’s social adjustment. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 74-101. Cummings, R. L., Vallance, D. D., & Brazil, K. (1992). Prevalance and patterns of psychosocial disorders in children and youth with learning disabilities: A service provider’s perspective. Exceptionality Education Canada, 2, 91-108. De Schipper, J. C., Tavecchio, L. W. C., Van Ijzendoom, M. H., & Van Zeijl, J. (2004). Goodness-of-fit in center day care: Relations of temperament, stability, and quality of care with the child’s adjustment. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 1 9, 257-272. Denham, S. A., Blair, K. A., DeMulder, E., Levitas, J ., Sawyer, K., Auerbach-Major, S., & Queenan, P. (2003). Preschool emotional competence: Pathway to social competence? Child Development, 74 (I), 238-256. Dubow, E. B., Tisak, J., Causey, D., Hryshko, A., & Reid, G. (1991). A two-year longitudinal study of stressful life events, social support, and social problem- solving skills: Contributions to children’s behavioral and academic adjustment. Child Development, 62, 583-599. Dunn, L. & Kuntos, S. (1997). Research In review: What have we learned about developmentally appropriate practice? Young Children, 52 (5), 4-13. Durkin, D. (1987). A classroom-observation study of reading instruction in kindergarten. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 2, 275-300. Educational Research Service. (1986). Kindergarten programs and practices in public schools. Arlington, VA: author. Elicker, J ., & Fortner-Wood, C. (1997). Research in review: Adult-child relationship in early childhood settings. Young Children, 51 (1), 69-78. Elkind, D. (1996). Young children and technology: A cautionary note. Young Children, 51, 22-23. Evangelista, N. J. (2002). Understanding the social and personal needs of the early school—aged child. In Carlson, J. F., & Waterman, B. B. (Eds.) Social and personality assessment of school-aged children: Developing interventions for educational and clinical use. (pp. 206-228). Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon. Feng, H., & Cartledge, G. (1996). Social skill assessment of inner-city Asian, African, and European American students. School Psychology Review, 25, 227-238. 62 Fowell, N., & Lawton, J. (1992). An alternative view of appropriate practice in early childhood education Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 7, 53-73. Gagnon, C., Craig, W. M., Tremblay, R. E., Zhou, R. M., & Vitaro, F. (1995). Kindergarten predictors in boys’ stable behavior problems at the end of elementary school. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 23, 751-766. Gresham, F. M., & Elliott, SN. (1990). Social skill rating scale. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Services. Goldenberg, C., Reese, L., & Gallimore, R. (1992). Context effects on the use of early literacy materials in Spanish-speaking children's homes. American Journal of Education Research, 100, 497-536. Goldsmith, E. B. (1999). Resource management for individuals and families. Minneapolis/St. Paul: West Publishing Company. Goody, E. N. (1997). Social intelligence and language: Another Rubicon? In A. Whiten & R. W. Byme (Eds), Machiavellian intelligence 11: Extensions and evaluations (pp. 364-396). Cambridge University Press. Greenwood, C. R., Walker, H. M., Todd, N. M., & Hops, H. (1979). Selecting a cost- effective screening device for the assessment of preschool social withdraw]. Journal of Applied Behavioral Analysis, 12, 639-652. Griffin, 8., Case, R., & Siegler, R. (1994). Rightstart: Providing the central conceptual prerequisites for first formal learning in arithmetic to students at risk for school failure. In K. McGilly (Ed.), Classroom lessons: Integrating cognitive theory and classroom practice (pp. 25-49). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Griffore, R. & Phenice, L. (2001). The language of human ecology. Iowa: Kendall/Hunt Publishing Company. Haapasalo, J ., Tremblay, R. E. (1994). Physically aggressive boys from ages 6 to 12: Family background, parenting behavior, and prediction of delinquency. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 62, 1044-1052. Hammond, W. R., & Yung, B. R. (1993). Psychology’s role in the public health response to assaultive violence among young African-American men. American Psychologist, 48 (2), 142-154. Hart, C. H., Burts, D. C., & Charlesworth, R. (1997). Integrated developmentally appropriate curriculum: From theory and research to practice. In C. H. Hart, D. C. Burts, & R. Charlesworth (Eds), Integrated curriculum and developmentally appropriate practice: Birth to age 8. New York: State University of New York Press. 63 Hart, C. H., Burts, D. C., Durland, M. A., Charlesworth, R., DeWolf, M., & Fleege, P. O. (1998). Stress behaviors and activity type participation of preschoolers in more and less developmentally appropriate classrooms: SES and sex differences. Journal of Research in Childhood Education, 12 (2), 176-196. Hartup, W. W. (1996a). Cooperation, close relationships, and cognitive development In W. M. Bukowski, A. F. Newcomb, & W. W. Hartup, (Eds), The company they keep: Friendship in childhood and adolescence (pp.213-23 7). New York: Cambridge University Press. Hartup, W. W., & Stevens, N. (1997). Friendships and adaptation in the life course. Psychological Bulletin, 121, 355-370. Hemmeter, M. L., Maxwell, K. L., Ault, M. J ., & Schuster, J. W. (2001). Assessment of practices in early elementary classrooms (APEEC). New York, NY: Teachers College Press. Hiebert, E. (1988). The role of literacy experiences in early childhood programs.- Elementary School Journal, 89, 161-171. Howes, C., & Ritchie, S. (2002). A matter of trust: Connecting teachers and learners in the early childhood classroom. New York, NY: Teachers College Press. Huffman, L. R., & Speer, P. W. (2000). Academic performance among at-risk children: The role of developmentally appropriate practices. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 15 (2), 167-184. Hyson, M. C., Hirsh-Pasek, K., & Rescorla, L. (1990). The classroom practices inventory: An observational instrument based on NAEYC’s guidelines for developmentally appropriate practices for 4- and 5- year-old children. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 5, 475-494. Jambunathan, S., Burts, D. C., & Pierce, S. H. (1999). Developmentally appropriate practices as predictors of self-competence among preschoolers. Journal of Research in Childhood Education, 13 (2), 167-174. J ipson, J. (1991). Developmentally appropriate practice: Culture, curriculum, connections. Early Education and Development, 2(2), 120-136. Jordan, N., Huttenlocher, J ., & Levine, S. (1992). Differential calculation abilities in young children from middle- and low-income families. Developmental Psychology, 28, 644-653. 64 Jones, I. & Gullo, D. F. (1999). Differential social and academic effects of developmentally appropriate practices and beliefs. Journal of Research in Childhood Education, 14 (1 ), 26-35. Keller, H. R. (1988). Children’s adaptive behaviors: Measure and source generalizability. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 6, 371-389. Kessler, S. A. (1991a). Early childhood education as development: Critique of the metaphor. Early Education and Development, 2, 137-152. Kessler, S. A. (1991b). Alternative perspectives on early childhood education. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 6, 183-197. Kessler, S. A., & Swadener, B. B. (Eds). (1992). Reconceptualizing the early childhood curriculum. New York: Teachers College Press. Knapczyk, D. R., & Rodes, P. (1996). Teaching social competence: A practical approach for improving social skills in students at-risk. Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole Publishing Company. ‘ Kontos, S., & Wilcox-Herzog, A. (1997). Teacher’s interaction with children: Why are they so important? Young Children, 52 (2), 4-12. Kostelnik, M. J ., Soderman, A. K., & Whiren, A. P. (2004). Developmentally appropriate curriculum: Best practices in early childhood education (3rd ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson. Kostelnik, M. J ., Whiren, A. P., Soderman, A. K., Stein, L. C., & Gre ory, K. (2002). Guiding children ’s social development: Theory to practice (4 ed.). Albany, NY: Delmar. Ladd, G. W., & Kochenderfer, B. J. (1996, April). Classroom peer acceptance, friendship, and victimization: distinct relational systems that contribute uniquely to children’s school adjustment. Paper presented at the American Educational Research Association Meeting, New York. Landy, S. (2002). Pathways to competence: Encouraging healthy social and emotional development in young children. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing Co. Lerner, J. V., & Lerner, R. M. (1983). Introduction. In Temperament and adaptation across life: Theoretical and empirical issues (Vol. 5, pp. 198-231): Academic Press. Lerner, J. V., Lerner, R. M., & Zabski, S. (1985). Temperament and elementary school children’s actual and rated academic performance: A test of a “goodness-of—fit” model. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 26, 125-136. 65 Lubeck, S. (1994). The politics of developmentally appropriate practice: Exploring issue of culture, class, and curriculum In B. L. Mallory & R. S. New (Eds), Diversity and developmentally appropriate practices (pp. 17-43). New York: Teachers College Press. Ludlow, B. L., & Berkeley, T. R. (1994). Expanding the perceptions of developmentally appropriate practice: Changing theoretical perspective. In B. L. Mallory & R. S. New (Eds), Diversity and developmentally appropriate practices (pp. 107118). New York: Teachers College Press. Marcon, R. (1992). Differential effects of three preschool models on inner-city 4 year- olds. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 7, 517-530. Marcon, R. (1995, April). Diflerential effects of preschool models on inner-city children: The “Class of 2000 ” transitions from third to fourth grade. Paper presented at the biennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Indianapolis, IN. McFall, R. (1982). A review and reformulation of the concept of social skills. Behavioral Assessment, 4, 1-33. Moisan, T. A. (1998). Identification and remediation of social skills deficits in learning disabled children. Unpublished master’s research paper. Chicago Stat University, Chicago, IL. Newcomb, A. F., & Bagwell,C. L. (1995). Children’s friendship relations: A meta- analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 11 7, 306-347. Newcomb, A. F., & Bagwell,C. L. (1996). The developmental significance of children’s friendship relations. In W. M. Bukowski, A. F. Newcomb, & W. W. Hartup, (Eds), The company they keep: Friendship in childhood and adolescence (pp.289-321). New York: Cambridge University Press. O'Loughlin, M. (1992, September). Appropriate for whom? A critique of the culture and class bias underlying developmentally appropriate practice in early childhood education. Paper presented at the Conference on Reconceptualizating Early Childhood Education: Research, Theory, and Practice, Chicago, IL. Parker, J. G., Asher, S. R. (1993). Beyond peer acceptance: Friendship adjustment and friendship quality as distinct dimensions of children’s peer adjustment In D. Perlman, & W. H. Jones (Eds), Advances in personal relationships (Vol. 4, pp. 261-294). London: Kingsley. Parker, J. G., Rubin, K. H., Price, J. M., & DeRoiser, M. E. (1995). Peer relationships, child development, and adjustment: A developmental psychopathology 66 perspective. In D. Cicchetti & D. Cohen (Eds), Developmental Psychopathology: Vol. 2. Risk, disorder, and adaptation (pp. 96-161). New York: Wiley. Pellegrini, A. D., & Glickman, C. D. (1990). Measuring kindergartener’s social competence. Young Children, 45 (4), 40-44. Prothrow-Stith, D. (1991). Deadly consequences. New York: Harper Collins. Raines, S. C., & Johnson, J. M. (2003). Developmentally appropriateness: New contexts and challenges. In Isenberg, J. P., & Jalongo, M. R. (Eds) Major trends and issues in early childhood education: Challenges, controversies, and insights. (pp. 85-96). New York, NY: Teachers College Press. Reynolds, A. (1989). A structural model of first-grade outcomes for an urban, low socioeconomic status, minority population. Journal of Educational Psychology, 81, 594-603. Rothbart, M. K., & Jones, L. B. (1998). Temperament, self-regulation, and education. School Psychology Review, 2 7 479-491. Santrock, J. W. (2000a). Children (6th ed.). Boston: McGraw Hill. Schachter, D., Pless, I. B., & Bruck, M. (1991). The prevalence and correlates of behaviour problems in learning disabled children. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 38, 323-331. Schweinhart, L., Weikart, D., & Lamer, M. (1986). Consequences of three preschool curriculum models through age 15. Early Childhood Quarterly, 1, 15-45. Schweinhart, L. J ., & Weikart, D. P. (1993). Success by empowerment: The High/Scope Perry Preschool Study through age 27. Young Children, November, 54-58. Shephard, L. & Smith, M. (1988). Kindergarten readiness and retention: A qualitative study of teachers’ beliefs and practices. American Educational Research Journal, 25, 307-333. Sherman, C. & Mueller, D. P. (1996, June). Developmentally appropriate practice and student achievement in inner-city elementary schools. Paper presented at Head Start’s third national Conference, Washington, DC. Soderman, A. K. (2002). PIOKS Early Readiness Social Competency Scale. Unpublished manuscript. Soderman, A. K., & Farrell, P. (2003). [2002-2003 Leap Frog Program]. Unpublished raw data. 67 Spodek, B. (1982). The kindergarten: A retrospective and contemporary view. In L. Katz (Ed.), Current topics in early childhood education (Vol. 4, pp. 173-192). Norwood, NJ: Ablex. Starkey, P., & Klein, A. (1992). Economic and cultural influences on early mathematical development. In F. L. Parker, R. Robinson, S. Sombrano, C. Piotrowski, J. Hagen, S. Randolph, & A. Baker (Eds), New directions in child and family research: Shaping Head Start in the 90s (p. 440-443). New York: National Council of Jewish Women. Steipek, D., Feiler, R., Daniels, D., & Milburn, S. (1995). Effects of different instructional approaches on young children’s achievement and motivation. Child Development, 66, 209-223. Steipek, D. J., & Ryan, R. H. (1997). Economically disadvantaged preschoolers: Ready to learn but further to go. Developmental Psychology, 33 (4), 711-723. Swanson, H. L. (1996). Meta-analysis, replication, social skills, and learning disabilities. Journal of Speczal Education, 30, 213- 221. Swanson, H. L., & Malone, S. (1992). Social skills and learning disabilities: A meta- analysis of the literature. School Psychology Review, 21, 427-443. Talwar, R., Nitz, K., & Lerner, R. M. (1990). Relations among early adolescent temperament, parent and peer demands, and adjustment: A test of the goodness of fit model. Journal of Adolescence, 13, 279-298. Taylor, A. R. & Machida, S. (1994). The contribution of parent and peer support to Head Start children’s early school adjustment. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 9, 387-405. Teglasi, H. (1997). Temperament. In G. G. Bear, K. M. Minke, & A. Thomas (Eds), Children ’s needs 11: Development, problems, and alternatives. Washington, DC: National Association of School Psychologists. Thomas, R. M. (2000). Comparing theories of child development (5th ed.). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth/Thomson Learning. Thomas, A., & Chess, S. (1977). Temperament and development. New York: Brunner/Mazel. Tremblay, R. E., Pagani-Kurtz, L., Masse, L. C., Vitaro, F. & Pihl, R. O. (1995). A bimodal preventive intervention for disruptive kindergarten boys: Its impact through mid-adolescence. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 63, 560-568. 68 Vallance, D. D., Cummings, R. L., & Humphris, T. (1998). Mediators of the risk of problem behavior in children with language learning disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 31, 160-171. Vaughn, B. E., Arizaa, M. R.,Krzysik, L., Caya, L. R., Bost, K. K., Newell, W., & Karura, K. L. (2000). Friendship and social competence in a sample of preschool children attending Head Start. Developmental Psychology, 36, 326-338. Vaughn, B. E., Colvin, T. N., Azria, M. R., Caya, L., & Krzysik, L. (2001). Dyadic analyses of friendship in a sample of preschool-age children attending head start: Correspondence between measures and implications for social competence. Child Development, 72 (3), 862-878. Walker, H. M., (1983). Problem behavior identification checklist (Revised). Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services. Walker, H. M., Severson, H., Stiller, B., Williams, G., Haring, N., Shinn, M., & Todis, B. (1988). Systematic screening of pupils in the elementary age range at risk for behavior disorders: Development and trail testing of a multiple gating model. Remedial and Special Education, 9, 8-14. Walsh, D. J. (1991). Extending the discourse on developmentally appropriateness: A developmental perspective. Early Education and Development, 2, 109-119. Waterman, B. B. (2002). Inforrnation-processing perspectives in understanding social and personal behavior. In Carlson, J. F., & Waterman, B. B. (Eds) Social and personality assessment of school-aged children: Developing interventions for educational and clinical use. (pp. 141-157). Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon. 69 lltr‘!) ll ll 1' ’1‘ .illlll l ’illllll Ill ll STAT llllllllljllgllllljlllllIlllll 504 0431 ll!