meats '// Jaw/1- a 03 3 #033 This is to certify hat the dissertation entitled MEDIATING AND MODERATING PROCESSES IN THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN SOCIOCULTURAL STRESS AND MENTAL HEALTH FOR LATINA/O STUDENTS AT A PREDOMINATELY WHITE UNIVERSITY presented by Duranda Cosette Orellana has been accepted towards fuifillment of the requirements for the PhD . degree' In PsychologL #1in Major Professor's Signature 75/3/100/ Date MSU Is on W We! Oppcrumlty Won - — —.-.-a--a-----n-o--n--n-.--.-.-.-.-A-.---.-o-o-n-a-n-o-c-----o---.- LIBRARY Michigan State University PLACE IN RETURN Box to remove this checkout from your record. To AVOID FINES return on or before date due. MAY BE RECALLED with earlier due date if requested. DATE DUE DATE DUE DATE DUE MAP 1 9 2006 0427 06 6/01 cJCIRC/DateDuepSS-ots MEDIATING AND MODERATING PROCESSES IN THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN SOCIOCULTURAL STRESS AND MENTAL HEALTH FOR LATINA/O STUDENTS AT A PREDOMINATELY WHITE UNIVERSITY By Duranda Cosette OreIIana A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fiIlfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Psychology 2004 \IE BET“ E' " it“; hipptrztx The s Stale [.. mea'uyh “35C02c ”1&ng . P V “ 9dr! T 34167 n ABSTRACT MEDIATING AND MODERATING PROCESSES IN THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN SOCIOCULTURAL STRESS AND MENTAL HEALTH FOR LATINA/O STUDENTS AT A PREDOMINATELY WHITE UNIVERSITY By Duranda Cosette Orellana This study examined a culture-specific adaptation of Taylor and Aspinwall’s (1996) model of Mediating and Moderating Processes in Psychosocial Stress with Latina/o undergraduate students at a predominantly White university. The adapted model included concepts of previous researchers who have tested the relationships between stressors, mediators, moderators, and mental health outcomes. Appraisal of sociocultural stress (i.e., acculturative stress, minority status stress), individual cultural characteristics (i.e., ethnic identity, acculturation level), perceived social support (i.e., perceived informal support, perceived formal support), and coping (i.e., direct and indirect coping) were hypothesized to predict mental health (i.e., wellbeing, distress) among Latina/o undergraduates. In addition several relationships among these constructs were hypothesized. The sample consisted of 201 Latina/o/Hispanic undergraduate students at Michigan State University. Students completed a questionnaire packet which included self-report measures of the various constructs in the model. Structural equation modeling (SEM) was conducted to test moderators and the hypothesized relationships in the adapted model. Post hoc analyses were conducted to improve the overall fit of the adapted model. The modified adapted model produced a good overall fit [)8 (df 167, N=201) = 241.67, p > 0.001, GFI = 0.90, AGFI = 0.86, RMSEA = 0.05, CFI = 0.94, NNFI = 0.93, dcmonv tires: Berth; \v frieze: I fizzai mod PNFI = 0.68, PGFI = 0.65]. Direct significant positive and negative relationships were demonstrated. Statistically significant indirect relationships were also demonstrated. Result revealed that neither gender nor SES moderated the relationships specified in the model. However, several relationships among the constructs in the model Significantly differed for heritage groups (i.e., mono-ethnic, bi-ethnic). For mono-ethnic participants, the relationship between individual cultural characteristics and wellbeing was found to be mediated only by appraisal of sociocultural stress, indirect coping, and distress. Perceived social support was found to promote both high and low levels of wellbeing for bi-ethnic participants but not for mono-ethnic participants. Analyses also revealed that there were group mean differences on several of the latent variables of the final model. This study focused on dimensions found to be salient for Latina/o ethnicity and culture as it expanded the literature on stress-mental health by being the first to empirically test the mediational processes by which individual cultural characteristics and perceived social support facilitate coping with sociocultural stress and consequently mental health. Findings suggest that university service providers consider and integrate contextual and ethnically relevant constructs into their service delivery. Furthermore, results indicate that for bi-mono-ethnic and bi-ethnic Latina/o individuals attending predominantly White universities identity development is a lifelong process. Copyright by Duranda Cosette Orellana 2004 'm“-v‘ .. bi...\ll.- . "mun ‘ s.n.ib$\‘ ACKNOWLEDGMENTS I wish to thank Robert Caldwell, Ph.D., Neal Schmitt, Ph.D., Israel Cuellar, Ph.D., and Fransisco Villarruel, Ph.D. for their direction and support which they provided during the process of conducting and completing this doctoral dissertation. I also wish to thank Seka Remsing for her assistance in the data collection stage. Finally, I wish to thank my fiancee, Marcel, and family for their emotional support throughout my graduate years. REY F4 {\TROZ CHAPI. LITER 1. Rea ' The RC"; TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES ........................................................................... x LIST OF FIGURES .......................................................................... xii KEY FOR THE MEASURMENT MODEL ............................................. xiii INTRODUCTION ........................................................................... 1 CHAPTER 1 LITERATURE REVIEW ................................................................... 6 Review of Stress-Illness Models ..................................................... 7 Life Events: Sources, Adaptations, and Outcomes .......................... 7 Integrative Stress and Coping Model .......................................... 10 The Stress Mediation Outcome Model ........................................ 13 Stress Mediation-Depression Model ........................................... 15 Model of Minority Status and Distress ........................................ 18 Model ofLatino College Student Adjustment......... 20 Model of Mediating and Moderating Processes in Psychosocial Stress ....................................................... 23 The Adapted Model ................................................................... 25 Review of the Constructs in the Adapted Model .................................. 3O Appraisal of Sociocultural Stress ................................................. 30 Appraisal ofMinority Status Stress 34 Appraisal of Acculturative Stress ......................................... 36 Mediating and Moderating Variables .......................................... 38 Individual Cultural Characteristics.................. 4O Acculturation Level41 Ethnic Identity... 44 Perceived Social Support . 46 Perceived Informal Social Support .................................. 49 Perceived Formal Social Support... 50 Coping ........................................................................ 51 Direct and Indirect Coping 54 Mental Health ..................................................................... 55 Distress. .. 55 Subjective Well- Being ...................................................... 57 Subjective Happiness .................................................. 57 Life Satisfaction.............................................................58 Integration of the Literature ....................................................... 59 Review of the Literature Supporting the Hypothesized Paths in the Proposed Model ..................................................................... 59 Path A: The influence of individual cultural characteristics on perceived social support ..................................... 60 vi CHAPI PATIO‘ H51? CHAPII IEIIICI PEI PTO-C IZS‘CZL'T‘! : Dee 3:: IWQ, PCV \. CHAPT] RESL'LI PE p5,, Path B: The influence of individual cultural characteristics on appraisal ofsociocultural stress. .....6...1 Path C: The influence of perceived social Support on appraisal of sociocultural stress ............................................ 62 Path D: The influence of individual cultural characteristics on coping... .............................................................................. 63 Path E: The influence of perceived social support on coping ........ 64 Path F: The influence of appraisal of sociocultural stress on coping ............................................................. 66 Path G: The influence ofcoping on mental health...68 CHAPTER 2 RATIONALE ............................................................................ 70 Hypotheses ................................................................................. 77 CHAPTER 3 METHOD .................................................................................... 79 Participants ............................................................................. 79 Procedure .................. . ............................................................. 80 Instruments ................................................................................... 84 Demographic Information Form ..................................................... 84 Appraisal of Cultural Stress ........................................................... 84 Minority Student Stresses Scale ................................................ 84 Social, Attitudinal, Familial, and Environmental Acculturative Stress Scale ................................................................. 85 Individual Cultural Characteristics .................................................. 86 Acculturation Rating Scale for Mexican Americans-Revised .............. 86 Multigroup Ethnic Identity Measure .......................................... 88 Perceived Social Support .............................................................. 89 Perceived Social Support from Family and Friends ......................... 89 Perceived Social Support from University Personnel ....................... 90 Coping ................................................................................... 90 The Brief Cope Inventory ....................................................... 90 Problem Focused Style of Coping .............................................. 93 Mental Health ........................................................................... 94 DSM Scale for Depression-26 ................................................... 94 Satisfaction with Life Scale ....................................................... 95 Subjective Happiness Scale ....................................................... 97 CHAPTER 4 RESULTS .................................................................................... 98 Phase One ................................................................................ 98 Phase Two .............................................................................. 98 Phase Three ............................................................................. 100 Individual Cultural Characteristics ARSMA-II ......................................................... 1 06 vii MEIM .............................................................. 107 Appraisal of Sociocultural Stress ....................................... 109 MSSS .................................................................... 109 S.A.F.E .................................................................... 112 Coping ....................................................................... 114 BCOPE .................................................................... 114 PF -SOC ................................................................. 116 Perceived Social Support ................................................. 118 PSS-F A .................................................................. 118 PSS-FR .................................................................. 120 PSS-UP .................................................................. 122 Mental Health .............................................................. 124 DSD-26 .................................................................. 124 SWLF ................................................................... 126 SHS ............................................................................ 129 Phase Four ............................................................................... 129 Results of the Hypotheses ........................................................ 132 Hypotheses 1-2 ............................................................... 132 Hypotheses 3-6 .......................................................... 133 Hypotheses 7-8 .......................................................... 134 Model I: Results of the overall adapted Latina/o student stress-mental health model ........................................... 134 Model 2 .................................................................. 135 Model 3 .................................................................. 138 Model 4 .................................................................. 142 Moderators .................................................................. 147 Gender ................................................................... 148 SES ...................................................................... 151 Heritage ................................................................. 155 CHAPTER 5 DISCUSSION ............................................................................... 163 Limitations ................................................................. l 69 Conclusions and Implications ........................................... 172 APPENDIX A. Demographic Information Form ....................................... 182 APPENDIX B. Minority Student Stresses Scale ........................................ 184 APPENDIX C. Social, Attitudinal, Familial, and Environmental Acculturative Stress Scale ................................................................ 186 APPENDIX D. Acculturation Rating Scale for Mexican Americans-Revised ...... 188 APPENDIX E. Multigroup Ethnic Identity Measure .................................... 190 viii APPENDIX F. Perceived Social Support from Family ................................. 191 APPENDIX G. Perceived Social Support from Friends ................................. 193 APPENDIX H. Perceived Social Support from University Personnel ................ 195 APPENDIX 1. The Brief Cope Inventory ................................................. 197 APPENDIX J. Problem Focused Style of Coping ........................................ 199 APPENDIX K. DSM Scale for Depression-26...... 200 APPENDIX L. Satisfaction with Life Scale .............................................. 203 APPENDIX M. Subjective Happiness Scale ............................................. 204 REFERENCES ................................................................................ 205 ix ILIL Iris: -4 r; . hi“ 1 . LIST OF TABLES Table 1. Demographic Information: Age, Year in College, and GPA of Participants ...................................................................... 81 Table 2. Demographic Information: Self Identification of Participants ............... 81 Table 3. Demographic Information: Place of Birth, Citizenship, and Residency of Participants ......................................................... 81 Table 4. Demographic Information: Participants families’ financial status compared to other students’ families at MSU .......................... 81 Table 5. Demographic Information: Ethnic/Racial Group Identified for Fathers and Mothers ................................................................ 82 Table 6. Demographic Information: Specific Ethnic/Racial Group Identified for Fathers and Mothers .............................................. 82 Table 7. Demographic Information: Place of Birth of Mothers and Fathers .......... 83 Table 8. Demographic Information: Level of Education of Mothers and Fathers. . .83 Table 9. Table 10. Table 11. Table 12. Table 13. Table 14. Table 15. Table 16. Table 17. Table 18. Intercorrelations and Reliability Coefficients for Reported Subscales of Measures ......................................................................... 101 Goodness of Fit Indices for Confirmatory Factor Analyses of Hypothesized Scales ......................................................... 103 Means, Standard Deviations, and Range of Derived Scales ............... 105 Acculturation Rating Scale for Mexican Americans-Revised (ARSMA-II) ..................................................................... 108 Multi- Ethnic Identity Measure (MEIM) ..................................... 110 Minority Status Stress Scale (MSSS) ....................................... 113 Social Attitudinal Familial & Environmental (S.A.F.E.) Acculturation Stress Scale ................................................... 115 The Brief COPE Inventory (Brief COPE) ................................. 117 Problem-Focused Style of Coping (PF-SOC) ........................... 119 Perceived Social Support from Family (PSS-FA) ........................ 121 Table I9 Perez" Table 20 Table: Table 32. T351623 Peres The D‘ lmerc E\pl0' Good“.- Mode} lnterce Model .. Imam; Mod 3} . ImeTCQ‘ " , IIIIETCO Interco Interco Inierm - Intercg Table 19. Table 20. Table 21. Table 22. Table 23. Table 24. Table 25. Table 26. Table 27. Table 28. Table 29. Table 30. Table 31. Perceived Social Support from Friends (PSS-FR) ........................ 123 Perceived Social Support from University Personnel (PSS-UP) ........ 125 The DSM Scale forDepreSSion-26 (DSD-26)............ 127 Intercorrelations and Reliability Coefficients for Scales Produced by Exploratory Factor Analysis .................................................. 130 Goodness of Fit Indices for Measurement and Structural Models ........................................................................... 136 Intercorrelations and Reliability Coefficients for Scales used in Mode12.... ........................................................................................ 139 Intercorrelations and Reliability Coefficients for Scales used in Model 4 ........................................................................... 144 Intercorrelations and Reliability Coefficients for Males .................. 149 Intercorrelations and Reliability Coefficients for Females ............... 150 Intercorrelations and Reliability Coefficients for Low SES ............. 153 Intercorrelations and Reliability Coefficients for High SES ............ 154 Intercorrelations and Reliability Coefficients for Mono-ethnic ......... 156 Intercorrelations and Reliability Coefficients for Bi—ethnic .............. 157 xi figure 1. Life Es Figcrel [mega Tigire 3. The St: Tigare4 Stress! Figure 5. Model Flgure6 Model Tigare'l Medial Figure 8. Adapts Figure 9 Ada?“ Figure 10 \Ieas Figure 11. Adar; Figure 12 RESpe Figure 13 Mode Fit." '7' Our 8 T4 Resp“ TIL”; " LIST OF FIGURES Figure 1. Life Events: Sources, Adaptations, and Outcomes ................... 9 Figure 2. Integrative Stress and Coping Model .................................. 12 Figure 3. The Stress-Mediation—Outcome Model .................................. 14 Figure 4. Stress-Mediation-Depression Model .................................... 17 Figure 5. Model of Minority Status and Distress ................................. 19 Figure 6. Model of Latino College Student Adjustment ........................ 22 Figure 7. Mediating and Moderating Processes in Psychosocial Stress ...... 24 Figure 8. Adaptation of Taylor & Aspinwall’s (1996) Model ................... 26 Figure 9. Adapted Model for Latina/o Students .................................... 27 Figure 10. Measurement and Structural Model .................................... 72 Figure 11. Adapted Model Results ................................................ 131 Figure 12. Respecified Model 2 Results .......................................... 140 Figure 13. Model 4 Results ......................................................... 145 Figure 14. Respecified Model for Bi-Ethnic Participants Results ............. 161 Figure 15. Respecified Model for Mono-ethnic Participants Results ......... 162 xii mjiLQLL‘ fl Minority Status Appraisal or Appraisal of Social Attitudinz Alien iron 3 Interperstm .- 2.34.. I ..‘c lejiv.gdal Cg..-.l Acculmration R; Latino Orie: Association \Iultigroup Ezhr. Reecgriiiicr Exploration Pinched Socia Remixed F. Family Inri: Perceived Socia. Receixed f: PTOVIdIng F Peer ClOSer Perceiyed SOCIa etched L ElalionSh The B ° Tie“ Icope ACTIVe C Seeking KEY FOR THE MEASURMENT MODEL Appraisal of Cultural Stress Minority Status Stress Scale (MSSS) Appraisal of Discrimination MSSSI Appraisal of Campus Culture MSSSZ Social, Attitudinal, Familial, and Environmental Acculturative Stress (S.A.F.E) Alienation due to Cultural Barriers SAFE] Interpersonal Stress SAFE2 Individual Cultural Characteristics Acculturation Rating Scale for Mexican Americans-Revised (ARSMA-II) Latino Orientation ARSMAl Association with Anglos ARSMA2 Multigroup Ethnic Identity Measure (MEIM) Recognition of Ethnicin MEIM] Exploration of Ethnicity MEIMZ Perceived Social Support Perceived Social Support from Family (PSS-FA) Received Family Support PSSFAI Family Intimacy PSSFA2 Perceived Social Support from Friends (PSS-F R) Received Peer Support PSSFRl Providing Peer Support PSSFR2 Peer Closeness PS SF R3 Perceived Social Support from University Personnel (PSS-UP) Providing University Personnel Support PSSUPl Received University Personnel Support PS SUP2 Relationship to University Personnel PS SUP3 Coping The Brief Cope Inventory (BCOPE) Active Coping BCOPE] Seeking Support BCOPEZ Passive Coping BCOPE3 xiii heilem Focus: Assertix : Aioidar \lerfial Health DSM Scale for I Emotion; Phisml '_ Sandal 5 Satistaction Su‘qiecriie IJ Problem Focused Style of Coping (PF-SOC) Assertive Coping PF SOCl Avoidant Coping PFSOC2 Mental Health DSM Scale for Depression-26 (DSD-26) Emotional Depressive Symptoms DSDl Physical Depressive Symptoms DSD2 Suicidal Symptoms DSD3 Satisfaction with Life Scale SWLS Subjective Happiness Scale SHS xiv Accord : ' A Striated at apt recent increase of the total pop; the majority. 63" Census. DELHI) .' minority groups American C can underrepresente educational gar significant disc Latinos are dis 0f non~Latina . OTLatinos had Saduating fig ‘Cabie'ra & X. emironllients educational SI For m and adjust”)?E that Latin 0 ‘ Latin“ “th INTRODUCTION According to the U. S. Census, the Hispanic or Latina/o population for 2000 was estimated at approximately 35 million or 12.5% of the total US. population, a 57.9% percent increase from 1990 estimates. In the state of Michigan, approximately 324,000 of the total population of 10 million reported being of Hispanic or Latina/o descent, with the majority, 68%, indicating that they were of Mexican heritage (U. S. Bureau of the Census, 2000). According to census reports, Latinos constitute one of the fastest growing minority groups in this country (U .8. Bureau of the Census, 2000). However, the American Council on Education (ACE; 2001) has reported that Latinos continue to be underrepresented in higher education. According to ACE (2001), although the educational gap between Latinos and non-Latina/o Whites has narrowed in recent years, significant discrepancies continue to exist. For example, the college completion rates for Latinos are disproportional when compared to non-Latina/o Whites. In 1998, 28 percent of non-Latina/o Whites ages 25-29 had earned a bachelor's degree while only 10 percent of Latinos had done so. Previous research indicates that the low rate of Latinos graduating from four-year colleges can in part be accounted by cultural incongruencies (Cabrera & Nora, 1994; Gloria & Robinson-Kurpius, 1996), nonsupportive university environments (Cabrera & Nora, 1994; Cervantes, 1988; Ponterotto, 1990), and educational stereotypes (Retish & Kavanaugh, 1992). For many students the transition into a university environment involves challenge and adjustment to the college atmosphere. However, findings of several studies indicate that Latina/o college students experience higher levels of stress in comparison to non- Latina/o White students (Bourassa, 1991; Cervantes, 1988; McCormack, 1995; 0c.- Lq addition to tin face ismes that ' These issues in; immigrant and t' ethnic or culture iP-hinney. I991. . Within th regarding their cr ot‘culzural incong silo grevt up in l Others of mainst. Latina'o student TIISI time may q acculturated an: Students must a} Gloria & Robin ime’Personal 1e- {Smedley et al ‘ diSCriiitinatiorr and intemalizati C“litir Ofientat Ponterotto, 1990; Quintana, Vogel, & Ybrarra, 1991; Smedley, Myers, & Harrell, 1993). In addition to the adjustments that must be made by all college students, Latina/o students face issues that many ethnic minority individuals living in the US. must contend with. These issues include: (a) experiencing racism and discrimination owing to their immigrant and/or minority status, (b) relating to the dominant culture, (c) retaining their ethnic or cultural heritage, and (c) experiencing Stress as a result of these experiences (Phinney, 1991; Smith 1991; Roysircar-Sodowsky, & Maestas, 2000). Within the university environment, Latina/o students may experience conflict regarding their cultural orientation (e. g., ethnic loyalty and cultural awareness) as a result of cultural incongruencies (Baron & Constantine, 1997). For example, Latina/o students who grew up in predominantly Latino neighborhoods for the first time must interact with others of mainstream culture on a consistent basis (Shibazaki, 1999). Conversely, Latina/o students who are raised in highly acculturated families and environments for the first time may question their orientation as they encounter Latina/o students who are less acculturated and hold different values and attitudes (Gloria & Rodriguez, 2000). Latina/o students must also negotiate an unwelcoming university environment (Cervantes, 1988; Gloria & Robinson-Kurpius, 1996). In particular, Latina/o students may experience interpersonal tensions between themselves and non-Latina/o White students and faculty (Smedley et al., 1993), negative events related to their minority status (e. g., prejudice, discrimination, multicultural insensitivity; Cabrera & Nora, 1994; McCormack, 1995), and internalization of these events (Retish & Kavanaugh, 1992). Thus, the differences in cultural orientations as well as experiences of negative events because of their minority status may be sources of stress for Latina/o students. Despite ~ . kl. lltformaclc l irdicatine that I. enter discrimi'.‘ 1 . . '1 Sudents- experie- rescar‘ chers (e g . stressors and me.“ “here the Latina Halon 1993. I RodrigueL Mien Villarreal. 1997. 7 relationships amt. asignificartt cone &tuthnesr, New ‘ the issues experie in the \Iidu est ar Diffient Study vs i1 “here Latina'o H PODulation (N = 4 \llchigan State Li RESearch ; these studies h3\‘e Despite studies (e. g., Bourassa, 1991; Cabrera & Nora, 1994; Cervantes, 1988; McCormack, 1995; Ponterotto, 1990; Quintana et al., 1991; Smedley et al., 1993) indicating that Latina/o students who attend predominantly White universities report greater discrimination and social and cultural alienation than non-Latina/o White Students, experience adjustment difficulties, and have higher dropout rates, few researchers (e.g., Najera, 1990) have attempted to identify the relationship between stressors and mental health outcomes with Latinos attending Midwestern universities where the Latina/o population has low enrollments. Most of the research (e.g., Aspinwall & Taylor, 1992; Morris, 1997; Padilla, Alvarez, & Lindholm, 1986; Quinones, 1996; Rodriguez, Myers, Monis, & Cardoza, 2000; Saldana, 1994; Shibazaki, 1999; Solberg & Villarreal, 1997; Suarez, Fowers, Garwood, & Szapocznik, 1997) that has examined this relationships among Latina/o college students has been conducted in states where there is a significant concentration of the Latina/o population: mainly in the West Coast, Southwest, New York, and Florida. Clearly, there is a need to research and understand the issues experienced by Latina/o college students at predominately White universities in the Midwest and the impact of these experiences on their mental health. Therefore, the present study will focus on Latina/o undergraduate students at Michigan State University where Latina/o/Hispanic enrollment was reported as 2% (N=859) of the total student population (N = 42,407) for the 2001/2002 school year (Office of Planning and Budgets, Michigan State University, 2002). Research investigating Latina/o students’ adjustment to college has not typically been the object of systematic study (Hurtado, Carter, & Spuler, 1996). Furthermore, these studies have not relied on a single definition of college adjustment. For example, some have defined adjustment as institutional commitment, good academic performance, and the absence of psychological distress (Chartrand, 1992). Others have conceptualized adjustment as students’ positive responses to unfamiliar norms, values, and expectations that predominate on campus (Bennet & Okinaka, 1990). Finally, some (Hurtado et al., 1996) have conceptualized adjustment as a multifaceted phenomenon characterized by resolution of psychological distress and transitional trauma. Studies specifically examining psychological functioning as a measure of Latina/o students’ adjustment to stressful experiences in college (e.g., Aspinwall & Taylor, 1992; Morris, 1997; Najera, 1990; Padilla et al., 1986; Quinones, 1996; Rodriguez et al., 2000; Saldana, 1994; Shibazaki, 1999; Solberg & Villarreal, 1997; Suarez et al., 1997) have found that various individual characteristics, social resources, cultural characteristics, and/or appraisal and coping processes are related to psychological functioning. For example, individual variables such as: self-efficacy (Solberg & Villarreal, 1997) self-esteem (Aspinwall & Taylor, 1992; Najera, 1990; Padilla et al., 1986), locus of control (Aspinwall & Taylor, 1992; Padilla et al., 1986), introversion/extroversion (Padilla et al., 1986), and optimism (Aspinwall & Taylor, 1992); external variables such as social support (Morris, 1997; Shibazaki, 1999; Solberg & Villarreal, 1997; Riggio, Watring, & Throckmorton, 1993) and community involvement (Riggio et al., 1993); cultural characteristics such as ethnic identity (Quinones, 1996; Shibazaki, 1999), acculturation level (Morris, 1997; Quinones, 1996; Saldana, 1994; Rodriguez et al., 2000), biculturalism (Suarez et al., 1997), generational status (Padilla et al., 1986), and gender role socialization (Quinones, 1996); and process variables such as appraisal and coping strategies (Aspinwall & Taylor , 1992) have all been found to be related to psychological outcomes for Latina/o students. he no “ 'rugh tint MI ages esults su: r I r 'ltu "II. CD. Hyr‘fi/ 5“: ‘ to l ‘ 'ionsh.p reia. Although none of the above studies have tested all these variables simultaneously, the results suggest that for Latina/o college students, individual characteristics, social support, cultural characteristics, appraisal, and coping responses have a complex relationship to each other that in turn affect mental health. The relay scientists for St identify the proc .. .. . . I being of mama- relationship bets. outcomes (\Iartir Stress on mental I 1906). This rese; betneen Stress ar 1997), These res 'ra'iance in outco iiariables that in‘ between the tn 0, ad potentially al CTilerion variable Siess moderator quantitative I i e Chapter 1 LITERATURE REVIEW The relationship between stress and mental health has been of research interest to scientists for some time. Investigators from a wide range of disciplines have attempted to identify the processes by which stressors produce harmfiil effects on the mental well- being of individuals (W arheit, 1979). Early research on stress focused on the simple relationship between stressfiil life events and various physical and psychiatric health outcomes (Martin, 1989). Thus, researchers originally conceptualized the impact of stress on mental health as a simple univariate process (Cervantes & Castro, 1985; Groag, 1996). This research had limited explanatory power, indicating that the relationship between stress and mental health outcomes was relatively weak (Cohen, Hettler, & Park, 1997). These results led researchers to attempt to account for greater proportions of variance in outcomes by examining models that focused increasingly on mediators (variables that influence both the predictor and criterion by accounting for the relations between the two; Lonner & Adamopoulos, 1997) and moderators (variables that control and potentially alter the strength or direction of the relationship between a predictor and criterion variable; Cohen et al., 1997; Groag, 1996; Holahan, Moos, & Bonin, 1997). Stress moderators and mediators examined included qualitative (i.e., gender, race) or quantitative (i.e., social support, personality) variables that are related to the extent to which a person will be affected by stress (Groag, 1996). Some researchers (e. g., Billings & Moos, 1982a, 1982b; Lazarus & F olkman, 1984) became interested in the process that takes place between the experience and outcome of a stressor. According to these researchers, too many of the stress-outcome models emphasized stable, structural properties of the person and environment rather than examining the changes that occur as the process of stress unfolded (Martin, 1989). Thus, in an attempt to outline the dynamics of the stress-mental health relationships, they began studying models that included not only stable factors but also changing processes that impacted mental health (Billings & Moos, 1982a, 1982b; Lazarus & Folkman, 1984; Miranda & Castro, 1985). These researchers were known as the transactional model theorists. Review of Strefiss—Illnesa Mode—ls The following section presents models of Stress from which the proposed model was developed for this study. The models presented below were developed by theorists interested in refining early stress-illness models which did not provide a theoretical understanding of the dynamic interactions among personal, environmental, and/or process variables. These investigators argued that most stress-illness models conceptualized these variables as “static” and “occurring within a closed system” (Warheit, 1979). Life Evenls: Sources. Adamfionsband Outcomes. Warheit’s (1979) Life Events, Sources, Adaptations, and Outcomes Model (Figure 1) posits that stressfirl life events arise from three sources: the individual’s psychological and biological constitution, culture, and social environment. Thus, psychosocial stress is conceptualized as being different fi'om the stressfiil events that precipitate it. The adaptive-nonadaptive screens are the coping resources available to individuals to meet stressfiil demands. These may include an individual’s personality, cultural beliefs, and social support networks. According to Warheit (1979), stress in this model is an altered state that occurs when the demands on an individual exceed his or her capabilities to respond. The outcomes of stress in Warheit’s model are viewed as individual symptoms, cultural syndromes and/or social dysfunction. According to Warheit (1979), his theory reflects the systemic nature of life events, coping resources, stress, and stress outcomes as they occur in a temporal context. Warheit (1979) hypothesized that when individuals face a stressful event, they will first rely on their individual characteristics to handle stress. Individuals will then rely on support from others. If both of these are found to be inadequate, they may then turn to their culturally provided beliefs and values. However, in practice, most individuals will seek to maximize on all available resources in a complementary manner. The importance of Warheit’s (1979) model to the present study lies in its consideration of culture in the stress-mental health process. According to Cuellar (2000), culture has been viewed as having a potential impact on numerous aspects of health, illness, and adaptation. More specifically, culture influences perceptions of illness, manifestations of illness, prevalence rates, susceptibility, acceptance of illness, reactions to illness, adjustment to illness, and its assessment and treatment (Cuellar & Gonzalez, 1999; Cuellar, 2000). Thus, culture is critical to the present study because it allows for the inclusion of culture-specific beliefs and values which are believed to influence the experience of stress. Culture is believed to give “individuals sources of explanation for events that cannot be accounted for by a society’s logic or science. Culture also provides symbolic definitions that attach meaning to events.” (Parsons, 1951; as cited by Warheit, 1979, p. 503). Also important to the present study is Warheit’s acknowledgment of the impact of personal, cultural, and social resources on stress and his conceptualization of 852 .xofiagv 88830 was .m=o_samc< .mooczom HEo>m as a 8sz m EEO U .H. DO cococémxm Eoom moEoéim mEo macaw m m mm a m wmmahm AxlUOmOEUme m m mm H m mcogchmmLO \ H 4/McoEmoam68m #8288 .3836 Z mcoEccoD 2850 1885825 stsm B5532 5:33:00 mzmmmum .toaasm 38m amoneam 32305 m>Em “coacoagcm 2830 A hmoumtoaofimau m BMW/m 38m _ 3:232: » b b 3958 stress as an altered state that is distinct from the stressfitl event (stressor) and stress outcome (distress). Although Warheit’s (1979) model incorporates personal, cultural, and the social environment as important factors in the relationship between stress and stress outcomes, it does not include process variables (i.e., appraisal, coping). Consequently, the model does not account for individual differences in the perception of Stress and coping methods. In addition, Warheit conceptualizes culture as existing outside of the individual. Thus, he fails to recognize that culture is also a system of ideals within an individual (Geertz, 1984) that can cause considerable within group difl’erences (Sodowsky, Lai, & Plake, 1991) and provides a better understanding of the person (Cuellar, 2000). Mtive Stress and Coping Model. Billings and Moos (1982a) proposed the Integrative Stress and Coping Model (Figure 2) which posits that the stress-mental health relationship is not only mediated by personal characteristics (i.e., self-confidence, self- estcem, optimism) and environmental resources (i.e., emotional support; guidance and assistance from one’s broader social network) but also by cognitive appraisal, coping responses and the interrelationships among these domains. According to Billings and Moos (1982a), personal characteristics and environmental resources are also directly related to subsequent fitnctioning. Furthermore, the personal system, environmental system, and characteristics of the stressors, directly influence the appraisal of and coping responses to stress which ultimately determine one’s health and functioning. In this model there is also a bi-directional relationship between personal and environmental resources. 10 The importance of the Integrative Stress and Coping Model to this study is that like Warheit (1979), Billings and Moos (1982a) emphasize a relationship between personal resources and social resources. Also like Warheit (1979), Billings and Moos (1982a) examine the impact of personal and social resources on the stressors. However, unlike Warheit (1979), Billings and Moos (19823) include appraisal and coping processes in their model. Thus, they consider the impact of personal and social resources on appraisal and coping responses. Although Billings and Moos (1982a) include process variables in their model, they conceptualize appraisal and coping as one component, reflecting an inseparable relationship. The implicit assumption is that a Single assessment of coping provides a representative sample of both appraisal and coping and that this is sufficient for evaluating the relationship of these processes to mental health outcomes (F olkman & Lazarus, 1986). Billings and Moos (1982a) also use the term “environmental resources” to refer to “social resources” (i.e., social support). Although the use of one term for the other is subtle, scientists are increasingly recognizing that social support is only one component of environmental resources, a much broader term that also encompasses variables such as income, neighborhood cohesion, job opportunities, etc. Furthermore, it is increasingly recognized that there are individual differences in the appraisal of social support and ability to extract needed support (Taylor & Aspinwall, 1996). These differences in appraisal of social support are likely to be affected by an individual’s cultural characteristics such as worldview. However, using Billings and Moos’ (1982a) model this relationship would be difficult to assess since they do not include the construct of culture in their model. 11 Amwi .802 a. 3556 6on 9:80 can $85 9583:: .N 8sz mmomoommm iEazzoEZm ozazofioiza izommmm mmngmmm mmommgm 972 0258 iezazzogm zoammémo oz< imam“? / mmUMDOmm—M iZOmmmm 12 The Stress-Mediation-Outcome Model. The model proposed by Miranda and Castro (1985) consists of seven interactive components: (1) Life change events or stressors; (2) structural factors (i.e., SES, acculturation level, gender) specific to the individual experiencing the life events change; (3) individually perceived stress which influences personal resources, coping responses, and support networks; (4) coping responses which are seen as being precipitated into action by individually perceived stress; (5) personal resources that mediate the effect of coping responses on mental health status; (6) support networks which mediate the impact of individually perceived stress on mental health; and (7) the outcome, mental health. See Figure 3. According to Miranda and Castro (1985) there is a bidirectional relationship between the level of individually perceived stress and mental health status. This relationship can also be mediated by coping responses, personal resources and social support networks. Although coping responses and personal resources are characterized as functioning independent of social support systems, all three have an effect on mental health status. The final component of Miranda and Castro’s (1985) model is the relationship between life events change and individual perceived stress. This relationship is believed to be mediated by structural components (e. g., SES, Age). Miranda and Castro’s (1985) model is important to this study because they attempt to define what specific events, under what conditions, are linked with what sorts of mental health outcomes, for what people. The authors’ argue that relating undifferentiated life change to an undifferentiated psychological outcome forms an overly simplistic model. Moreover, a simple model suggests a simple intervention. 13 Amma— dBmaU 8 «ESQ/c E52 oEooSO-=o:m%oE-mmobm 2? .m ocswfi 353qu toqasm mmobm @88me .283 l r Bioeom maco>m 352 i i 2.32%.: owaao a: owe. .e ...................... xom .m $883M Ecofiom _o>oi_ ........... 0 III... =2§838< .N moms—093m wanU mmm . H rrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrr rrrnrrrrrirrrrrrrurrirrr 833:3 33323 moist”; Houseman wcuflco—Z Eugene?— l4 Like Warheit (1979), Miranda and Castro (1985) consider culture in their model by including structural factors (i.e., acculturation) in their model. Unlike Billings and Moos (1982a), Miranda and Castro (1985) clearly delineate appraisal and coping as separate events because each is believed to serve different firnctions. For example, the appraisal of an event is believed to involve an evaluation of costs. The appraisal process then determines what type of coping response is necessary. Unlike Warheit (1979) and Billings and Moos (1982a), Miranda and Castro (1985) do not delineate pathways indicating the direct impact of personal and social resources on either the stressor or the appraisal of the stressor(s). Although, they posit a bi-directional relationship between coping responses and personal resources, they do not consider a direct relationship between support networks and coping responses. Miranda and Castro (1985) like Warheit (1979) acknowledge the role of culture in their model by including structural factors specific to the individual, but they combine demographic (i.e., gender, SES) and cultural variables (i.e., acculturation level) into one component (i.e., structural factors). Thus, they fail to recognize that cultural variables involve a dynamic process, whereas as demographic variables are merely descriptive (Saldana, 1994). Finally, like Warheit (1979) Miranda and Castro (1985) conceptualize culture as existing outside the individual or separate from an individual’s characteristics. Stress-Mediation-Depression Model. Leyva (1990) proposed an adaptation of Billings and Moos’ (1982a) model specifically for Mexican/Mexican—American women (Figure 4). Leyva was interested in examining the impact of acute stressors (i.e., death of a loved one, marriage, divorce) and chronic stressors (i.e., cultural conflict, marital conflict, occupational/economic conflict) on mental health. She included acculturation 15 and socioeconomic status as personal mediators, neighborhood cohesion and perceived social support as environmental mediators, method of coping and focus coping as coping processes, and depressed mood as the outcome variable. Leyva hypothesized a bi- directional relationship between personal and environmental mediators. In addition, she hypothesized that these mediators each directly influenced stressors, appraisal of stressors, coping strategies, and depression. Furthermore, she hypothesized direct relationships from stressors to appraisal of stressors, appraisal of stressors to coping strategies, and coping strategies to depression. Leyva’s (1990) model is important to this study because it considers the impact of personal and social resources on Stress (W arheit, 1979; Billings & Moos, 1982a), appraisal of stress, and coping responses (Billings & Moos, 1982a). Furthermore, like Miranda and Castro (1985), Leyva (1990) conceptualizes appraisal of stress and coping Strategies as two distinct processes. Like Warheit (1979) and Miranda and Castro (1985), Leyva (1990) includes culture in her model by incorporating cultural conflict as a chronic stressor and acculturation level as a personal resource, both prominent components in the stress-mental health relationship for Mexican American women. However, unlike Warheit (1979) and Miranda and Castro (1985), Leyva (1990) acknowledges that culture cannot be separated from the individual and includes acculturation level as a personal resource. Leyva (1990) fails to distinguish between social resources and environmental resources when she includes social support and neighborhood cohesion as environmental mediators, Furthermore, by including SES and acculturation level as personal resources, 16 ENVIRONMENTAL MEDIATORS Social Support PERSONAL MEDIATORS SES Acculturation Level STRESSORS V 7 APPRAISAL Chronic Stressors Acute Stressors COPING STRATEGIES L DEPRESSION Figure 4. Stress-Mediation-Depression Model (Leyva, 1990) 17 she also fails to distinguish between demographic and dynamic cultural variables. In addition, similar to Warheit (1979), Leyva (1990) conceptualizes stress as “the appraisal of acute and chronic stressors and the evaluation of whether or not these stressors exceed or tax the individual’s ability to cope” (p. 28). Therefore, by including the variable, appraisal of chronic and acute stressors, she introduces a redundant variable in her model. Finally, although this model has been adapted and tested Specifically with Mexican American women its generalizability to other Latino groups such as Latina/o students is limited. Model of Minority Status and Distress. Saldana (1994) presented a model that provides an understanding of the relationship between stressors faced by Latina/o students at predominantly Anglo universities and psychological distress (Figure 5). Saldana (1994) hypothesized that for Latina/o Students, the relationship between precursor variables (i.e., social class, gender, ethnicity) and psychological distress was mediated by personal resources (i.e., acculturation level), college-related stress common to all university students (e.g., role strains; stress resulting fiom tension or conflict between the obligations and expectations associated with one role versus another), and stresses more relevant to Latina/o students (e.g., minority status stresses). Saldana’s (1994) model is important to this study because it is one of a few stress- illness models designed specifically for Latina/o college students. In addition, Saldana (1994) distinguishes between precursor variables and acculturation so that the relationship between ethnicity (i.e., ethnic group membership) and acculturation could be examined. “For Latinos, this implies the relevance of acculturation level as a dynamic 18 9&9 acme—ma $0me 98 £58m 5:052 CO 682 .m eBwE mmmdhm 233.5 mAOM 2822.8 $95 m>E§DSDoo< .3583. 252:5 =a 9 Engine $803 $85 of 5 28m 2255 =a 8 038:3“ $805 $23 of 5 23m mAr < ma < B mommobm €552 coup—3303‘ E 3855 Romme—onoxmm Q $30ch 20% 03:00 2V $8585 19 variable separate from purely descriptive variables such as ethnicity or social class.” (p. 125). Like Warheit (1979), Miranda and Castro (1985), and Leyva (1990), Saldana (1994) considers culture in her model by focusing on acculturation level as a personal resource and minority status stress and acculturative stress as stressors faced by Latina/o students within the university environment. By including acculturation level as a personal resource as does Leyva (1990), she does not separate culture fiom the individual as do Warheit (1979) and Miranda and Castro ( 1985). In addition, like Warheit (1979), Billings and Moos (19823) and Leyva (1990) she considers the impact of personal resources on chronic Stressors. Although Saldana’s (1994) model was designed for Latinos at predominantly Anglo universities, it was tested at a university in the West coast. In addition, Saldana (1994) does not directly measure ethnic identity or acculturative stress as does Leyva (1990). Rather, she assesses ethnic identity from a combination of items of an acculturation scale and infers acculturative stress from pathways connecting demographic variables, level of acculturation, college stresses, minority stresses, and psychological distress (Rodriguez et al., 2000). Furthermore, her model does not include variables that the other models reviewed include such as social resources (Warheit, 1979; Billings & Moos, 1982a; Miranda & Castro, 1985; Leyva, 1990) and process variables like appraisal and coping processes (Billings & Moos, 1982a; Miranda & Castro, 1985; Leyva, 1990). Model of Latino College Student Adjustmgrl. Rodriguez et a1. (2000), unlike Saldana (1994), proposed a model that distinguishes the impact of stress as a result of level of acculturation, the process of acculturation, and minority-status stress. The purpose of the model was to explain the impact of generic college stresses, minority- 20 status stresses and acculturative stresses on psychological well-being and psychological distress, beyond that attributable to gender, socioeconomic status, ethnicity, level of acculturation, and academic self-confidence. See Figure 6. More specifically, Rodriguez et a1. (2000) distinguished between the impact of minority-status stress and generic college-student stresses on psychological outcome. They also distinguished among the impact of stresses as a result of acculturation (i.e., stresses that originate in the process of acculturation; perceived cultural incompatibilities, cultural self-consciousness), those attributable to level of acculturation (stresses impacted by and individual’s level of acculturation; psychological and somatic Stress), and minority-status stress (i.e., experiencing discrimination on the basis of being minority). The importance of Rodriguez et al.’s (2000) model to this study is that like Warheit (1979), Miranda and Castro (1985), Leyva (1990), and Saldana (1994), the researchers include the concept of culture by considering acculturation level, acculturative stress, and minority status stress. Like Saldana (1994), Rodriguez and colleagues distinguish between demographic or precursor variables and acculturation. However, unlike Saldana (1994), the researches distinguish and separately measure acculturative stress and minority status stress. Like Saldana (1994), this model does not contain variables included in the above models such as social resources (Warheit, 1979; Billings & Moos, 1982a; Miranda & Castro, 1985; Leyva, 1990) and appraisal and coping processes (Billings & Moos, 1982a; Miranda & Castro, 1985; Leyva, 1990). In addition, unlike Saldana’s (1994) model, this model was created to examine the relationship between stress and psychological 21 808 ...a a SEES causes? 283m 09:8 oeado 382 a 2&5 338232 ESE—Ema :02 A 83.85 822232 ESE—Ema T 3:83—83 mommobm magmectocaz mEonzoB ..... coco . __ . 2.200 32365»; bow 22.03% 8325 09:00 25:00 GOCNHS£BOO< _8_mo_o:o>mm A _. % mMm 22 adjustment of Latinos attending colleges where the student body is primarily non-White and Latinos constitute the largest group. Model of Mediatingand Moderating Process in Psychosocial Stress. Taylor and Aspinwall (1996) proposed a model described as a set of nested models that draws on comprehensive approaches (e.g., Ensel & Lin, 1991; Lazarus & Folkman, 1984; Billings & Moos, 1982a). See Figure 7. The model depicts personal resources, external resources, social support, appraisals, as mediators of the relationships between stress and coping. They suggest a bi-directional relationship between personal and external resources. Personal and external resources are hypothesized to directly affect the stressor itself, appraisals of the stressor, and coping. Personal resources are believed to directly influence the availability, mobilization, and maintenance of social support. Social support in turn is hypothesized to be directly related to appraisals and coping. Finally, direct relationships from stressors to appraisal, appraisals to coping and coping to psychosocial outcomes as hypothesized. The importance of Taylor and Aspinwall’s (1996) model to this study is that unlike any of the previous models reviewed, they distinguish between external resources and social support. They describe external resources as aspects of the individual’s environment that shape the demands and the situation (e. g., time, money, environmental conditions). In doing so, they recognize that social support is in part an internal resource since there are individual differences in how one perceives and extracts social support. Therefore, they consider the direct effect of differences in personal characteristics on social support. Like Billings and Moos (19823), they consider process variables in their model but distinguish between appraisal and coping as do Miranda and Castro (1985) and 23 380330 BBSonozmm Goa .=§>E%< a. 813.5 mmobm _a_oomo:9$m 5 8330.5 mamas—co: wee @3332 s euswi toaqsm Eoom mama—cu t Seamed 329.com £83534 < common—m mooSomom EESRm 24 Leyva (1990). Like Billings and Moos (1982a) and Leyva (1990) they consider the direct effect of social, external, and personal resources on the stressor, appraisal of the stressor, and coping responses. However, unlike Warheit (1979), Miranda and Castro (1985), Leyva (1990), Saldana (1995) and Rodriguez et al (2000), Taylor and Aspinwall (1996) do not consider the concept of culture as part of their model. Furthermore, the model in its present form has not been tested with any population. The Adapted Model. The model tested in this study was an adaptation of Taylor and Aspinwall’s (1996) Model of Mediating and Moderating Processes in Psychosocial Stress (see Figure 8) that incorporated concepts from Warheit’s (1979) Life Events, Sources, Adaptations, and Outcomes Model, Billings and Moos’ (1982a) Integrative Stress and Coping Model, Miranda and Castro’s (1985) Stress-Mediation-Outcome Model, Leyva’s (1990) Stress-Mediation-Depression Model, and two models developed specifically for Latina/o college students; Saldana’s (1994) Model of Minority Status and Distress, and Rodriguez et al.’s (2000) Model of Latino College Student Adjustment. See Figure 9. Slavin, Rainer, McCreary, and Gowda (1991) indicated that any theoretical model that adds additional detail to existing models runs the risk of being too complex, too difficult, less feasible, and less understandable. Thus, they suggested creating specific culture-relevant dimensions of each component of an existing model without adding whole new components. In light of this suggestion, the adapted model was an attempt to incorporate aspects of Latino culture into each dimension of Taylor and Aspinwall’s (1996) model. However, for practical and conceptual reasons, this study examined only a 25 880230 368233 .282 68: m.=§=am< a 53¢ do 8:332 .w 2%: toaqsm 300m A V ll wcaoo magma? A mooBOmom Ecofiom 26 3509:; 038035 28 8053 $8350 _m_00mono>mm $265 as use @500 353% 2.253 8m .082 8233‘ .o oSwE saga 23 3:52: toaqnm 300m 33088 +< 3:53 2:5 _o>oq =o_§2_=8t€23oo< Ea $0.5m £58m btocg mmobm 3323208 mo 28353 27 portion OfT that whurai content \ aiitl cultural grot. recommende' messors that Latinos As stated' distinct from t specifically. tl an individual. social suppon Because this s Charm eri st it s hemeen other this Stud}; onl} e’h’imined. Sir. conceptualized as exceeding hi only if it Was portion of Taylor and Aspinwall’s ( 1996) model. Slavin et al (1991) also recommended that culturally relevant dimensions focus on measurement issues, particularly questions of content validity. Thus, some of the most salient stressful events for people in a given cultural group should be included in the assessment of stressors. To address this recommendation, this study focused on minority status stress and acculturative stress, two stressors that are prominent in the lives of many members of oppressed groups including Latinos. As stated above, Taylor and Aspinwall (1996) conceptualized social support as distinct from external resources (i.e., income level, education, opportunities, etc.) More specifically, they viewed social support as an external resource that is in part affected by an individual’s personal characteristics. For example, how one perceives and extracts social support may be influenced by factors such as one’s self-esteem or locus of control. Because this study is concerned with social support, only the direct effect of personal characteristics on social support was examined. Thus, the bidirectional relationship between other external resources and personal resources was not included. Finally, in this study only the appraisal of the stressors and not the stressors themselves were examined. Similar to Warheit (1979) and Leyva (1990), stress in this model was conceptualized as an altered state that occurs when an individual appraises the stressors as exceeding his or her capabilities to respond. Thus, stress was assumed to be present only if it was appraised as a negative event. For example, although two students may experience the same event (e. g, cultural self-consciousness, discrimination) one may find it very stressful while the other dismisses it. Therefore, in theory, the event is likely to 28 have a negative impact only on the individual who appraises the event as stressful (Landrine & Klonofi‘, 1996) Taylor and Aspinwall’s (1996) model was adapted to Latina/o students at a Midwestern university where the student body is predominantly White. Like Billings and Moos’ (1982a), Miranda and Castro’s (1985), and Leyva’s (1990) models, this adaptation of Taylor and Aspinwall’s (1996) model is dynamic in nature because it included process variables such as appraisal and coping. Thus, the indirect effects of individual cultural characteristics (e. g., acculturation level, ethnic identity) and perceived social support (e.g., perceived formal and informal support) on mental health (e. g., wellbeing, distress), the direct effect of individual cultural characteristics and perceived social support on the appraisal of cultural stress (e.g., acculturative stress, minority status stress) and coping strategies (e.g., direct and indirect coping), and the direct effect of individual cultural characteristics on perceived social support were examined (Taylor & Aspinwall, 1996). The adapted model also examined two chronic stressors that Saldana (1994) and Rodriguez et al. (2000) reported as salient for many Latina/o college students; acculturative stress and minority status stress. In addition, like Rodriguez et al. (2000) the adapted model distinguished between the impact of stresses that originate in the process of acculturation and stresses that result from a student’s minority-status. Like Warheit (1979), Miranda and Castro (1985), Leyva (1990), Saldana (1994), and Rodriguez et al. (2000), this model examined the role of culture in the relationship between stress and mental health by including aspects of Latino ethnicity such as: (a) acculturation level (i.e., cultural values, attitudes, and behaviors); (b) ethnic identity (i.e., the subjective sense of ethnic group membership); (c) acculturative stress (i.e., stresses 29 that originate in the process of acculturation and include perceived cultural incompatibilities and social self-consciousness); and (d) minority status stress (i.e., the experiences associated with minority status that include powerlessness, discrimination, and prejudice). Review of the Constructs in the Adapted Model Appraisal of sociocultural stress. Stress is a concept that has been defined in many ways. Some regard stress as a stimulus or condition that produces a change of some sort. Others define stress as a turbulent reaction or response (Lazarus & Launier, 1978). Finally, there are those who believe that there is limited explanatory power in these two definitions of stress for they do not describe the interaction of the person- environment which can mediate the impact of stressful experiences. Thus, researchers such as Lazarus and Folkman (1984) defined stress as a person-environment encounter that is appraised as relating to one’s well-being, and taxes or exceeds the person’s resources to cope with the situation. They conceptualized stress as existing not just in the environment but also within the person. Furthermore, the appraisal of stress determined how a person reacts. For example, if environmental demands are appraised as exceeding a person’s resources, the result is distress or feeling vulnerable and/or fearing that one’s well-being is endangered (Lazarus & F olkman, 1984; Lazarus & Launier, 1978). Folkman and colleagues (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984; Lazarus & Launier, 1978) thus make a distinction between stressful events (stressors) and the appraisal of those StI'CSSOI'S. 3O Stressors have been conceptualized as discreet or acute events resulting in a short- run response that is highly contained and situation bounded or as chronic (i.e., accumulation of negative events) resulting in a response pattern that emerges slowly over time that can develop into a prevailing state (Cohen et al., 1997; Pearlin 1993). Pearlin, Lieberman, Menaghan, and Mullan (1981) and Kessler, Price, and Wortman (1985) defined acute or discrete stressors as acute life events and focused on the role of major life events on mental health outcomes. Major life events (i.e., graduation, marriage, divorce) were considered acute events because they were time-limited and required some type of change on the part of the individual. Conversely, chronic stressors were considered life events which persisted continuously over time (e. g., marital discord, financial difficulties) and were not initiated by a discreet event (Cohen et al., 1982). Past research indicates that there is empirically weak evidence to support a direct association between discrete stressors and psychological well-being (Pearlin, 1993). Due to this fact, some researchers (e.g., Lazarus and colleagues, Pearlin and colleagues) began to concentrate on chronic stressors. Their work demonstrated that chronic stressors were better predictors of psychological distress (Billings & Moos, 1984; Lazarus, 1984; Pearlin et al., 1981). However, each of the researchers defined chronic stressors differently. For example, Lazarus and colleagues defined chronic stressors as daily hassles or irritating, fi'ustrating, distressing demands (e. g., trouble relaxing, losing things, not enough time for family) that to some degree characterize everyday transactions with the environment (Kanner, Coyne, Schaefer, & Lazarus, 1981). Pearlin and colleagues conceptualized chronic stressors as role strains or chronic strains or stressors that arise 31 from the social roles (e. g., spouse, parent, and worker) that pe0ple adopt (Pearlin & Schooler, 1978). Early stress-outcome researchers were not only interested in assessing the relationship between discreet stressors and mental health but also in examining the effect of the number of stressors on stress outcomes. However, this produced confounds between the predictor, mediator variables, and criterion (Martin, 1989). Thus, researchers began to examine the relationship between specific chronic stressors, personality variables, social support, coping style, and stress outcomes (Martin, 1989). Although the examination of specific chronic stressors and psychological outcomes proved to be more promising (Vega, Warheit, & Meinhardt, 1985) in recent years, this research has been overshadowed by a growing interest in the effect of cumulative discreet stressors on stress outcomes (Pearlin, 1993). The various conceptualizations of stress and stressors have affected the measurement of these constructs and ultimately research results. For example, certain stress-outcome researchers (e. g., Saldana, 1994; Rodriguez et al., 2000; Warheit, 1979) have tested models that only examine stressors or the number of stressfirl events which people experience. However, other researchers (e. g., Billings & Moos, 1982a; Miranda & Castro, 1985; Leyva, 1990) have examined models that include individuals’ appraisals (evaluations) of events and situations as stressful. Several researchers have found that even when stressors and the appraisal of these stressors have been included in stress- mental health models, the latter has been found to be a better predictor of distress (i.e., depression, physical symptoms) (Cohen, 1986; Cohen, Kamarck, & Mermelstein, 1983). 32 In light of past research indicating that the appraisal of stress is a better predictor of stress outcomes, in this study stress was conceptualized as an individual’s appraisal of a stressor as stressful. Thus, like Lazarus and Folkman (1984), a distinction between the stressor itself and the appraisal of the stressor is implied. Naturally, the implication is that the fiequency of the stressor is assessed, however, only the appraisal of the stressor is included in the model as a predictor. Furthermore, because the study of specific chronic stressors is more promising, this study focused on the appraisal of two chronic stressors that have been found to be stressful for many Latinos living in the United States: minority status stress and acculturative stress. Research has shown that Latina/o students differ in their experience of racist events and the acculturative process (Rodriguez et al., 2000; Saldana, 1994). These appraisal differences are expected to have varying impact on stress outcomes. Acculturative stress and minority status stress are ongoing, culturally, specific stressors or “chronic role strains” in the life of many Latinos (Morris, 1997) that are above and beyond the generic stressors (e. g., financial difficulties, academic problems) experienced by all college students (Smedley et al., 1993). Past research indicates that the process of acculturation can be a source of stress that can lead to negative psychological outcomes or psychological distress (Cuellar, 2000). More recently, researches have begun to demonstrate that the experience of minority status and events that are directly related to the unique customs, values, and beliefs of ones culture are also sources of stress (Slavin et al., 1991; Utsey, 1998). Despite these findings, few studies (e. g., Rodriguez et al., 2000; Saldana, 1994) have attempted to understand the impact of sociocultural stress (stress resulting from the process of acculturation and from minority 33 status or cultural group affiliation) on the mental health of Latinos. Clearly, there is a need to research and understand this relationship. Appraisal of Minority Status Stress. All groups are subject to experience negative stereotypes, prejudice, and discrimination. However, ethnic minority groups or groups with less power and/or status are more likely to experience these negative events (F iske, 1993) on a consistent basis. Landrine and Klonoff (1996) have conceptualized the experience of negative stereotypes, prejudice, and discrimination as minority status stress. According to the authors, minority status stress is culturally specific stress or the experience of negative events that happen to minorities because they are minorities. Thus, they contend that theoretical models and lines of investigation from generic stress research can be applied to the study of minority status stress. The impact of minority status stress on individuals has been documented in various ways. For example, negative experiences related to one’s minority status have been implicated in the development of several psychiatric disorders (i.e., substance abuse and depression [Burke, 1984; Pillay, 1984; as cited in Utsey, 1998]), low self-esteem (Simpson & Yinger, 1985; Smith, 1985; as cited in Utsey, 1998), and lower levels of life satisfaction (Broman, 1997; as cited in Utsey, 1998). However, despite these findings, the research indicates that the relationship between minority status stress and psychological outcomes is not simple, direct, or absolute, but rather, it varies along a number of dimensions (Chavira & Phinney, 1991). Thus, how minority individuals appraise minority status stress varies and its impact on psychological health is moderated by various factors such as social support and individual characteristics, (Landrine & Klonoff, 1996; Phinney, 1996; Sarason & Sarason, 1984). Nevertheless, even when the 34 impact of these factors is considered, minority status stress has been found to have a greater negative impact than life events or daily hassles do on the physical and mental health of minorities (Landrine & Klonoff, 1996). Several researchers have found that students of color enrolled at predominantly White universities ofien experience minority status stress due to stereotyping, prejudice, and discrimination in either blatant or subtle forms (Balenger, Hoffman, & Sedlacek, 1992; McClelland & Auster, 1990; Pascarella, Edison, Nora, Hagedorn, & Terenzini, 1996; Schwitzer, Griffin, Ancis, & Thomas, 1999; Stone & Archer, 1990). On these university campuses, minority status stress has been reported as the result of academic stereotyping, pressure to conform to stereotypes, and/or prejudice in the form of limited respect and unfair treatment by faculty, teaching assistants, and students (Ancis, Sedlacek, & Mohr, 2000). Characteristics which have been found to increase the likelihood that minority students experience actual or perceived prejudice, stereotyping and/or discrimination include: minority group membership, time spent at the university, residence status (McCormack, 1995), psychological sensitivity, vulnerability to the campus social climate, and interpersonal tensions with White students and faculty (Smedley et al., 1993). Most of the research that has examined students’ experiences of prejudice, stereotyping, and discrimination has investigated differences between White students and racially-ethnically heterogeneous groups of students (Ancis et al., 2000). These studies have consistently shown that minority status stress is greater for Afiican American students than it is for Whites and other minorities (Ancis et al., 2000; Cabrera & Nora; 1994; Hurtado, 1992; McCormack, 1995; Smedley et al., 1993). Few researchers (e. g., 35 Morris, 1997; Rodriguez et al., 2000; Saldana, 1994) have examined within group differences in the relationship between minority status stress and psychological outcomes with Latina/o students. However, these studies indicate that this relationship varies as a firnction of Latina/o students’ acculturation level, ethnic identity (Morris, 1997; Rodriguez et al., 2000; Saldana, 1994), comfort with their own cultural values, unique historical background, comfort with individuals who are culturally different, adjustment experiences (Ancis, et al., 2000; Hurtado et al., 1996), and coping styles (Phinney & Chavira, 1995; Schmader, Major, & Gramzow, 2001). Appraisal of Acculturative Stress. In the past, the concept of acculturative stress has been confounded with minority status stress. However, stresses originating from one’s minority status (i.e., experiencing discrimination on the basis of being minority) are different fiom stresses resulting from the process of acculturation (i.e., stresses associated with negotiating between two or more cultural groups) (Rodriquez et al., 2000). Acculturative stress for many Latinos involves language difficulties, perceived cultural incompatibilities, cultural self—consciousness (Gil, Vega, & Dimas, 1994; Padilla, Cervantes, Maldonado, & Garcia, 1988) and commitment or lack of commitment to culturally prescribed values/behaviors (i.e., familisrn, cultural pride) (Vega, Zimmerman, Gil, Warheit, & Apospori, 1993). Although acculturative stress and minority status stress are significantly correlated (Sanchez & Fernandez, 1993), one is not inherently part of the other. They are theoretically and empirically distinct with acculturative stress making an independent contribution to psychological distress (Rodriguez et al., 2000). However, like minority status stress, acculturative stress can also result in an increased risk for mental health-related problems (Roysircar-Sodowsky & Maestas, 2000). Furthermore, its 36 impact is mediated by a number of variables such as social support, cognitive attributes such as appraisals and attitudes toward acculturation, and the degree of tolerance for and acceptance of cultural diversity (Berry & Kim, 1988; Williams & Berry, 1991 ). Although acculturative stress is a common experience for first-generation immigrants, U.S.-born, second and later generation ethnic minorities can also experience acculturative stress in response to pressure to maintain ethnic ties or conflicts that arise out of bicultural socialization (Roysircar-Sodowsky & Maestas, 2000). For example, within the university environment, peer group influences, along with the constant bombardment of White societal values and standards, are likely to erode retention of ones culture (Sue & Sue, 1990). The problem becomes one of conflict in knowing how to balance participation in two different cultures with different values, beliefs, and expectations for behaviors (Cervantes, 1988; F iske, 1988). The outcome of one holding values highly divergent from those of the majority culture may result in feelings of malintergration (Loo & Rolinson, 1986) or bicultural conflict experienced in the form of sociocultural alienation (i.e., sense of personal discontinuity that occurs as a result of disruption in cultural patterns), cultural confusion (i.e., inability to identify and associate with a definite norm within a given context when confronted with multiple norms), and cultural conflict (i.e., perceiving one’s values and beliefs as incompatible with a given social interaction) (Kiefer, 1974). The educational system is a vehicle for the acculturation process and serves as a source of acculturative stress (Cuellar, 2000) for many Latina/o college students. Like most students, Latina/o students face challenging academic and social conditions in higher education (Fuertes & Westbrook, 1996; Mena, Padilla, & Maldonado, 1987). 37 However, for many Latina/o students, college is a time when they must examine and modify some of their long-held beliefs and attitudes, particularly with regard to interacting with culturally different individuals (Constantine & Baron, 1997). Furthermore, they may encounter numerous and simultaneous changes in their relationships, routines, and/or ideas about self, work, family, health, and/or economics (Schlosser, 1990). These environmental and internal demands can tax or exceed Latina/o students’ adaptive resources (Monat & Lazarus, 1991). As a result, they may experience acculturative stress or sociocultural alienation and their sense of well-being may be challenged (Albrecth & Adelman, 1987; Loo & Rolison, 1986). Indeed, researchers have found that for many minority students, experiences of sociocultural alienation or acculturative stress may result in temporary academic difficulties, personality disintegration, emotional uncertainty, anxiety, depression, psychosomatic symptoms, suicidal ideation, and dropping out of school (Allen, Amason, & Holmes, 1998; Fuertes & Westbrook, 1996; Hovey & King, 1996; Kim, 1995; Loo & Rolison, 1986; Rodriguez et al., 2000; Williams & Berry, 1991). However, researchers have also found that the relationship between acculturative stress and mental health for Latina/o undergraduates varies as a fimction of factors such as perceived social support (Hovey & King, 1996) and individual characteristics such as acculturation level (Saldana, 1994; Sanchez & Fernandez, 1993; Szapocznik, Santisteban, Kurtines, Perez-Vidal, & Hervis, 1984; Zane & Mak, 2000). Mediating and Moderating Variables. Although early stress researchers obtained reliable correlations between stressors and mental health, these associations were disappointingly modest in magnitude, on average accounting for less than 10% of the 38 variation in distress (Turner & Roszell, 1994). Thus, researchers (e. g., Warheit, 1979) began to search for factors that might better explain this relationship and account for more of the variation in stress. Mediator and moderator variables or variables that affect the experience of stress and what its effects (Taylor & Aspinwall, 1996) were introduced into stress research to account for more of the variance in outcomes. Researchers found that individuals’ mental health outcomes typically depended upon two broad classes of variables: environmental resources and personal characteristics (Dean, 1986). Some researchers (e.g., Billings & Moos, 1982a, 1982b; Lazarus & Folkman, 1984; Pearlin & Schooler, 1978) emphasized “process” and not just “static,” unidirectional models of stress. Thus, they included process (e. g., coping) and static (e.g., individual characteristics, environmental resources) variables in their models. They hypothesized that what an individual does (e. g., coping responses) could be important in mediating or moderating the impact of stress. They also believed that a process oriented or transactional approach to the study of stress and outcomes yielded more information regarding usefirl intervention strategies as it is easier for people to change what they do then to change their personality traits or social environments (Martin, 1989). The inclusion of process and static variables in the same model has been essential in understanding the relationship between stress and mental health. For example, researchers have found that personal and environmental resources not only have a direct influence on one’s mental or physical health (Billings & Moos, 1982a, 1982b), but they also directly influence the stressor itself (Ensel & Lin, 1991), the appraisal of stress, and one’s reliance on specific coping strategies (Cohen & Edwards, 1989; Taylor & Aspinwall, 1996). Although no single study can incorporate every potential interaction 39 between process and static variables or foresee every confounding factor (Vega et al., 1985), the more we know about how these variables and how their interactions affect the stress-mental illness process, the more accurately we will be able to target interventions and the more effectively we will be able to design intervention strategies (Hough, 1985). This study incorporated various static and process variables in order to understand the relationship between sociocultural stress and mental health for Latina/os undergraduate students at a predominantly White university. Below is a review of the various mediating and/or moderating variables that were included in the adapted model. Individual culturaLchagtcteri stics. In the past, the literature has been inherently concerned with the effects of individual personality characteristics on the relationship between stress and mental health. Researchers interested in understanding the impact of individual characteristics on mental health have investigated various personality characteristics including: negative affectivity (Watson & Clark, 1984), pessimistic explanatory style (Peterson, Seligman, & Vaillant, 1988), hardiness, (Kobasa, 1979), Optimism (Scheier & Carver, 1985), psychological control (Bandura, 1977), self-esteem (Whisman & Kwon, 1993), self-confidence (Holahan & Moos, 1987, 1991), and ego strength (Worden & Sobel, 1978). These personality characteristics have been found not just to contribute to psychological well-being but also to the occurrence of a stressor (Fame, Sebellico, Gnugnoli, & Corallo, 1992; Rhodewalt & Zone, 1989) provision of social support (Cohen, Sherrod, & Clark, 1986; Dunkel-Schetter, Folkman, & Lazarus, 1987) appraisal of stressors (Campbell, Chew, & Scratchley, 1991; Jerusalem, 1993; Rhodewalt & Zone, 1989) and coping responses (Aspinwall & Taylor, 1992; Holahan & Moos, 1987; Jerusalem, 1993) 40 Despite the fact that individual cultural characteristics (i.e., worldview, values, beliefs) provide an understanding of the person (Cuellar, 2000) and like personality characteristics cause considerable within group differences on mental health outcomes (Sodowsky et al., 1991), few have paid explicit attention to their role in the relationship between stress and mental health status (Miranda & Castro, 1985; Slavin et al.,1991). Thus, most researchers have failed to acknowledge that one’s personality and culture are inextricably intertwined (Lonner & Adamopoulos, 1997). That is, they do not recognize that culture does not simply exist at the macro level, but also exists at the micro level or as a conceptual structure or system of ideals within an individual (Geertz, 1984). Thus, the concern in this study was with two individual cultural characteristics that have been found to influence the relationships between stress and mental health; acculturation and ethnic identity (Rodriguez et al., 2000; Saldana, 1994; Sarason, Sarason, & Gurung, 2001; Taylor & Aspinwall, 1996). Acculturation level. Acculturation is generally viewed as an ecological, transactional process of cognitive, emotional, behavioral, perceptual, and ideological change that occurs as a consequence of a continuous, first-hand contact of two or more distinct cultural groups (Cuellar, 2000; Roysircar-Sodowsky & Maestas, 2000). Acculturation is a multifaceted construct that is composed of multiple factors in which people demonstrate varying degrees of strengths/weaknesses, capacities, and abilities. Acculturation is not only an exogenous process it also involves cultural changes at the individual psychological level (Cuellar, Siles, & Bracamontes, 2002; Marin, 1992). Acculturation has been considered by theorists as either a unidimensional or multidimensional process (Szapocznik et al., 1984). Unidimensional theorists assume 41 that change in cultural identity takes place along a single continuum over the course of time. More specifically, acculturating individuals are seen as being in a process of relinquishing the attitudes, values, and behaviors of their culture of origin while simultaneously adopting those of the new society (Marin, 1992; Ryder, Alden, & Paulhus, 2000). Multidimensional theorists assume that acculturation involves assimilation to the majority culture and retention of the minority culture Marin, 1992; Rogler, Cortes & Malgady, 1991). Theorists who adopt a multidimensional perspective argue that acculturation can be more completely understood when heritage and A mainstream cultural identities are seen as being relatively independent of one another. Thus, individuals may adopt many of the values and behaviors of the mainstream culture without giving up their self-identity developed in their culture of origin (Ryder et al., 2000). Furthermore, acculturation may involve a degree of assimilation to a total cultural context comprised of various cultural groups (Szapocznik et al., 1984). Although each of these models has its own assumptions concerning what happens to a person as he or she undergoes the process of acculturation, the models are not mutually exclusive. Each one of them may represent an adequate explanation for a person’s experience as he or she acquires competency in a new culture. However, they emphasize different aspects of the process of acculturation (LaFromboise, Coleman, & Gerton, 1993). Unfortunately, acculturation’s multidimensional nature has precluded ever having one measure capable of adequately and sufficiently capturing it (Negy & Woods, 1992). Currently, most measures assess one or two facets of acculturation at a time in either Mexican Americans or Cubans (Marin, 1992). Furthermore, there are acculturation measures that assess superficial changes brought about by contact with different culture 42 or experience with cultural objects, measures that assess more significant changes in an individual’s behavior (e.g., language use), and very few measures that attempt to assess changes in values and norms (Marin, 1992). Acculturation has also been difficult to assess because it occurs at different rates and along different developmental pathways for each individual, due to such factors as age at time of immigration, generational status, geographical location, personal motivations for assimilating into the dominant culture, schooling experience, and degree of contact with members of the majority group, other groups, and/or more acculturated members of their same ethnic group (Baron, 1991; Cuellar, Arnold, & Maldonado, 1995; Cuellar, Nyberg, Maldonado & Roberts,1997; Perez & Padilla, 2000; Szapocznik & Kurtines, 1980). Nevertheless, researchers have consistently found that ethnic behaviors and practices of immigrants tend to decline over time (Perez & Padilla, 2000; Sodowsky et al., 1991). Because it has been empirically demonstrated that Latina/os born in the United States are more likely to have higher levels of depression symptoms than immigrants (Burnam, Hough, Karno, Escobar, & Telles, 1987; Mosicki, Locke, Rae, & Boyd, 1989), it has been hypothesized that second generation highly acculturated Latinos are presumed to be at risk for mental health problems due to their exposure to more culturally based conflicts and internalization of negative stereotypes (Rogler et al., 1991). However, researchers have also found that acculturation relates linearly both negatively and positively (Cuellar & Roberts, 1997) and also curvilinearly to psychological distress (Cuellar, Roberts, Romero, & Leka, 1999; Rogler, et al., 1991). Thus both high and low scores on acculturation may lead to poor or good mental health. The empirical evidence for any of these relationships is contradictory and inconclusive. Different studies support 43 different relationships, and some studies even support more than one (Gil et al., 1994; Rogler et al., 1991). These contradictory results point out the need to examine empirically the relationships between cultural change and psychological adjustment by examining for acculturation level and acculturative stress simultaneously (Gil et al., 1994). Indeed, acculturation level has been found to mediate the relationship between stress and mental health, influence the appraisal of potential stressors, and affect the selection of coping strategies (Morris, 1997; Phinney, 1995; Phinney, Chavira, & Williamson, 1992; Phinney, Williamson, & Chavira, 1990; Roysircar-Sodowsky & Maestas, 2000; Ruiz, 1990). Ethnic identity. Closely related to the construct of acculturation is the notion of ethnic identity. Like acculturation, ethnic identity is a complex multidimensional construct that varies across members of a group (Phinney, 1996). However, acculturation and ethnic identity are relatively independent constructs that are both experienced by ethnic minorities. The construct of ethnic identity differs from the construct of acculturation in that acculturation is a response to the dominant group or is broadly concerned with the degree to which dominant cultural norms are accepted, rejected, or transformed by ethnic minorities, while ethnic identity is a response to one’s ethnic group and refers to attitudes, beliefs, and feelings toward one’s own ethnic group (Sodowsky & Lai, 1997). Thus, an ethnic minority individual feels both a push to acculturate to the dominant society and a pull toward one’s ethnic group GKoysircar—Sodowsky & Maestas, 2000). Although shifts in acculturation level bring about changes in ethnic identity development and vice versa (Baron, 1991), the two constructs are not inversely related (Torres, 1999; Velez, 1995). In fact, the research suggests that high ethnic identity 44 accompanied by a positive mainstream orientation is related to positive psychological outcomes, whereas high ethnic identity without at least some adaptation to the dominant culture may be problematic (Phinney, 1995). Ethnic identity is a concept that is only applicable in the context of multicultural societies and is essentially irrelevant in monocultural societies (Roysircar-Sodowsky & Maestas, 2000). In the United States, a multicultural society, ethnic identity is central to the self-concept of individuals from ethnic groups (Phinney, 1991). It is a part of the self-concept that is derived from an individual’s knowledge of belonging to a social group (or groups), together with the perceptions, knowledge, values, behaviors, and emotional significance attached to that group membership (Phinney, 1991; Tajfel &Tumer, 1979). Ethnic identity comprises a number of different components, including self-labeling, a sense of belonging, positive evaluation, preference for the group, ethnic interest and knowledge, and involvement in activities associated with the group (Phinney, 1991,1995) The research of ethnic identity development suggests that ethnic identity can be conceptualized as a process; individuals progress from an early stage in which one’s ethnicity is taken for granted, on the basis of attitudes and opinions of other or of society; through a period of exploration into the meaning and implications of one’s group membership; to an achieved ethnic identity that reflects a secure, confident sense of oneself as member of a group (Phinney, 1996). Individuals progress along these stages for various reasons. For example, Latina/o students who study the history of their ethnic group can lead to change in ethnic identity level (Constantine & Baron, 1997). Evidence of movement fi'om one ethnic identity stage to another may manifest itself in the ethnic 45 labels used by students. Thus, in the beginning stages of ethnic identity, students are likely to use terms such as Hispanic or Mexican-American which are considered neutral, widely accepted labels. As students advance to higher stages, labels indicating clear affiliation with group heritage such as Chicano(a), Latino(a), or Hispano(a) may be used (Baron, 1991; Baron & Constantine, 1997). Different stages of ethnic identity have been found to have different mental health correlates (Phinney & Kohatsu, 1997). Although the psychological implications of ethnic identity vary with changes in one’s identification (Phinney, 1996), the research suggests that a bicultural identity (an adaptive dual identification with Latino and American cultures) is associated with the best mental health outcomes (Bautista de Domanico, Crawford, & Wolfe, 1994; Szapocznik et al., 1984). Perceived Social support. Social support is a multidimensional construct (Sarason, Pierce, & Sarason, 1990; Sarason, Sarason, & Pierce, 1990) with dimensions that include social networks, received social support, and perceived social support. Social support networks refer to the system of significant others with whom people have social ties and that may be called upon for help in times of need (Barrera, 1986; Sarason, Sarason et al., 1990; Hobfoll & Vaux, 1993; Vaux, 1988). Social support has also been conceptualized as received support or supportive behaviors that members of the social network perform when they render assistance to an individual in need (Barrera, 1986; Sarason, Pierce et al., 1990). Perceived social support is defined as the degree to which individuals perceive and interpret supportive interactions or psychological and non- psychological resources as being available to them from their social network (Cohen & McKay, 1985: Cohen et al., 1986; Sarason, Pierce, & Sarason, 1994; Vaux, 1988). 46 Perceived support is assumed to be independent of the support network (i.e., an individual may report a large network but might not perceive supportive behaviors from those individuals), antecedent to both the stressor and psychological distress, and relatively stable over time (Eckenrode & Wethington, 1990; Lepore, Evans, & Schneider, 1991; Sarason et al., 1994). Although there are different dimensions to the construct of social support, they all tend to identify themes around feeling cared for and supported (Hobfoll & Vaux, 1993). Furthermore, all the dimensions of social support are viewed as stable resources from which an individual draws on in order to handle a stressor (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984; Lepore et al., 1991; Thoits, 1986; Thoits, 1995). Indeed, the literature indicates that when individuals are faced with a stressor(s), social support has been found to buffer the potentially negative impact of stress (Caplan, 1974; Cassel, 1974; as cited in Solberg & Villarreal, 1997). Researchers have found that social support is also associated with good mental health outcomes, by buffering the negative influences of stressfirl events and depression (Briones et al., 1990) and facilitating psychological and physical well-being (Rodriguez, 1998). However, not all researchers have found a positive effect of social support on mental and/or physical health, in fact, some researchers have found it to be related to poor mental health outcomes; suggesting that the relationship between social support and mental health outcomes is considerably complex (Vega, et al., 1985). The conflicting results have been found to depend in part on the dimension of social support that is assessed. For example, when social networks are used as predictors of mental health outcome, an effect on well-being but not distress is generally the result. Similarly, support received has been found to be a poor predictor of well-being or 47 distress. However, perceived social support has been found to most consistently contribute to well-being and distress (Dunkel-Schetter & Bennett, 1990; Procidano, 1992, Procidano & Smith, 1997; Sarason, et al., 1994). In particular, perceived emotional support (i.e., beliefs that love and caring, sympathy and understanding and/or esteem and value are available from significant others) has been found to be associated with better physical and mental health when individuals are faced with negative life events and chronic strains (Thoits, 1995). In general, the literature indicates that individuals who perceive that social support is available will have less difficulty responding to stressful episodes than individuals who do not perceive social support is available (Solberg & Villarreal, 1997). As the literature in the area of perceived social support has evolved, researchers have begun to suspect that it is important to take the source of support into consideration, and that whether assistance is perceived as supportive depends upon the environment in which social support takes place and who is providing the help (Valle & Bensussen, 1985; Wortman & Dunkel-Schetter, 1987). However, the research focusing on this issue has been concerned solely with differentiating among support or help from family members and close fiiends (Barling, MacEwen, & Pratt, 1988). This trend deemphasizes and devalues the benefits associated with more impersonal relationships that arise in formal settings (Adelman, Parks, & Albrecth, 1987). In response to these suggestions, this study intends to extend previous research by using a more comprehensive definition of social support. Thus, in the present study, Demaray and Malecki’s (2002) definition of perceived social support will be used. The researchers defined perceived social support as an individual’s perceptions of general or specific support (i.e., emotional, 48 informational), available or acted on, from people in their social network (e. g., family, peers, university personnel). Perceived informal social support. Support provided by primary and extended family has been defined as informal social support (Valle & Bensussen, 1985). Because Latinos adhere to familismo, a strong sense of family centrality and importance, great emphasis is placed on reciprocity and on strong emotional ties between family members (Cervantes & Castro, 1985). Family is thus seen as a primary source of social support manifested by (a) providing material and emotional support, (b) relying primarily on family members for help and support, (c) using family members as referents for attitudes and behavior, and (d) placing the needs of the family or family members before individual needs (Knight, Bernal, Garza, & Cota, 1993; Sabogal, Marin, Otero-Sabogal, VanOss-Marin, & Perez-Stable, 1987). Several researchers have examined Latino’s perception of family support in times of stress (Keefe, 1980; Keefe, Padilla, & Carlos, 1979; Moore, 1970; Murillo, 1976). However, few studies have examined Latina/o students’ perception of family support and its relationship to psychological adjustment. These studies indicate that among Latina/o students perceived availability of family support is associated with positive outcomes (i.e., psychological adjustment, academic adjustment) for students while perceived unavailability of family support is associated with negative outcomes (Arellano & Padilla, 1996; Hurtado et al., 1996; Lambom et al., 1997; Solberg &Villarreal, 1997; Terenzini et al., 1994). Latina/o students have been found to rely more on family support than their Anglo counterparts (Saldana, 1988). Furthermore, perceived family support and family role models have been found to be crucial in promoting college adjustment 49 and persistence among Latinos (Cardoza, 1991; Cooper, Jackson, Azmitia, & Lopez, 1998; Lango, 1995). In light of this research, Miranda (1980) has cautioned against assuming that the family is the only support system available for Latinos. Friends and community support systems play significant roles in providing support. Similarly, Valle and Vega (1980) advocate the necessity of looking beyond the extended family as the only source of support available. Perceived forrnal social support. Support from professors and other university personnel can be sources of formal support for Latina/o university students (Cooper et al., 1998; Valle & Bensussen, 1985). These individuals can not only serve as primary sources of social support, they provide help and access to information not otherwise attained (Arellano & Padilla, 1996; Quevedo-Garcia, 1987). However, due to the paucity of Latina/o faculty (Aguirre & Martinez, 1993) and administrators (de los Santos & Rigual, 1994) at US. universities, there are few individuals that Latina/o students can look to as role models for support (F iske, 1988; Verdugo, 1995). Faculty to student ratio for Latinos has been reported as 1 to 76 compared with a ratio of l to 24 for White students (Hispanic Association of Colleges and Universities, 1995). Researchers that have examined perceived formal social support among Latinos have focused on primary and secondary school students (Alva, 1991; Demaray & Malecki, 2002; Furlong, Chung, Bates, & Morrison, 1995). These studies indicated that perceived formal support (e.g., peers, teachers) was positively associated with adjustment and school-related outcomes. Studies that have focused on Latina/o college students have found that support from university personnel, (e.g., specific professors, counselors, 50 financial aid staff, student support services) are salient sources of support for Latina/o students (Gandara & Osugi, 1994; Hurtado et al., 1996; Loo & Rolison, 1986; Lopez, 1995; Young, 1992) that are positively associated to psychological adjustment or well being and adaptation to the college environment (Arellano & Padilla, 1996; Solberg, 1990). Furthermore, perceptions of the university environment and participation in university networks have been found to be significant predictors of adjustment and college persistence (Crouse, 1985; Gloria & Robinson-Kurpius, 1996; Oliver, Rodriguez, & Mickelson, 1985). Qpipg. The vast literature on stress and coping indicates that people have many distinct responses to stress (Holahan, Moos, & Schaefer, 1996). Although there have been several efforts to distinguish conceptually and empirically among the many different responses people may have to stress (Zeidner & Endler, 1996), to date, consensus about these response categories has not been achieved (Miller & Kaiser, 2001). Nevertheless, Compas and colleagues (Compas, Connor-Smith, Saltzman, Thomsen, & Wadsworth, 2001; Connor-Smith, Compas, Wadsworth, Thomsen, & Saltzman, 2000) suggest that the most fundamental distinction between different responses to stress is between voluntary coping responses and involuntary responses. According to the authors this distinction emphasizes the fact that not everything an individual does in response to stress constitutes coping. Thus, they reserve the term coping for conscious voluntary efforts in response to stressfirl events or circumstances. Previous researchers defined coping as a multidimensional process that involves conscious cognitive, behavioral, and emotional efforts to deal with internal and/or external demands of stressful events (Carver & Scheier, 1994; Carver, Scheier, & 51 Weintraub, 1989; Cohen et al., 1982; Folkman, 1984; Folkman & Lazarus, 1980, 1986; Lazarus & Folkman, 1984; Parker, Endler, & Bagby, 1993; Pearlin & Schooler, 1978). Conscious behavioral, cognitive, and emotional coping efforts have several different functions, including (1) the modification of the stressor itself, (2) alteration of one’s own evaluation or appraisal of the stressor in order to reduce perceptions of threat, and (3) management of one’s somatic or emotional reactions to the stressor (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984; Martin, 1989; Moos & Schaefer, 1993; Pearlin et al., 1981; Pearlin & Schooler, 1978). Coping is perceived as being process-oriented because it is said to involve the interaction of person and environmental factors and a reciprocal transaction between conscious emotional reactions, cognitive appraisals, and behavioral responses (Folkman, Schaefer, & Lazarus, 1979). Most research on stress and coping draws upon Lazarus and Folkman’s (1984) model of stress, appraisal, and coping. From this perspective, people appraise both the stressfirl demand and their available coping resources. On the basis of this appraisal, people tend to employ two broad but distinct coping strategies to reduce the psychological distress associated with the stressfirl demand: problem-focused coping and emotion-focused coping. Problem-focused coping is aimed at problem solving or doing something to alter the source of the stress (Carver & Scheier, 1994; Lazarus & F olkman, 1984; Moos & Schaefer, 1993). It may consist of altering situational circumstances behaviorally (behavioral problem-focused coping) through direct actions on the environment or on the self to remove or alter circumstances appraised as threatening. Situations may also be altered cognitively (cognitive problem-focused coping) through reinterpretation of existing circumstances or distracting oneself from stressfirl cues 52 (Thoits, 1986). An alternative to problem-focused coping is emotion-focused coping which is aimed at reducing or managing the emotional distress that is associated with (or cued by) the situation (Carver & Scheier, 1994; Lazarus & Folkman, 1984; Moos & Schaefer, 1993). Emotion-focused coping may consist of actions (behavioral emotion- focused coping) or thoughts (cognitive emotion-focused coping) to control the undesirable feelings that result from stressful circumstances (Thoits, 1986). A second major conceptual approach in the coping literature has been to divide coping into approach versus avoidant activities (e.g., Maddi, 1980; Roth & Cohen, 1986; Suls & Fletcher, 1985; as cited in Heppner, Cook, Wright, & Johnson, 1995). Approach activities are oriented toward confronting the problem while avoidant activities are oriented toward reducing emotional tension (Holahan & Moos, 1987). Some have suggested that approach and avoidance activities are simply metaphors for problem- focused coping and emotion-focused coping (Roth & Cohen, 1986). However, there is evidence indicating that affective or emotional responses may play important roles in problem-focused coping (Heppner & Krauskopf, 1987). Similarly, emotion-focused strategies although often oriented toward avoiding dealing with the source of stress can also be oriented toward approaching stressful circumstances (Holahan & Moos, 1987). In addition to researchers’ disagreements regarding the optimal conceptualization of coping (Rohde, Lewinsohn, Tilson, & Seeley, 1990), the study of cultural differences has also led to some questions about the widely used coping nomenclature (e.g., problem- focused vs. emotion-focused, approach vs. avoidant) as evident of a Western European bias for problem-focused and approach coping over emotion—focused and avoidant coping (Cross, 1995; Lee & Liu, 2001; Weisz, Rothbaum, & Blackburn, 1984). Thus, Cross (1995) advocates for the adoption of a more neutral terminology to study coping strategies among ethnic minority groups, since individuals are thought to have the adaptive capacity not only to utilize several strategies when faced with stressors, but also are capable of varying their coping responses with each situation (Billings, Cronkite, & Moos 1983; Cohen et al., 1982; Folkman & Lazarus, 1980). Cross suggests that the terms direct coping and indirect c0ping are preferable to either problem or emotion- focused coping or approach vs. avoidant coping. Direct aad Indirect Coping. Cross (1995) defines direct COping as the use of strategies designed to actively mange, resolve, or influence stressful demands through one’s own efforts (e. g., problem solving, support seeking). Direct coping is believed to predominate when situational demands seem controllable or people feel that something constructive can be done (Billings et al, 1983; Carver et al., 1989; Folkman, 1984; Folkman & Lazarus, 1980; Thoits, 1995). Indirect coping is defined as the use of strategies designed to adjust to stressfirl demands by changing the self rather than the situation (e. g., accepting the situation, self-distraction) (Cross, 1995). Indirect coping is more likely when demands seem uncontrollable and people feel that the stressor is something that must be endured (Billings et al, 1983; Carver et al., 1989; Folkman, 1984; Folkman & Lazarus, 1980; Thoits, 1995). A considerable amount of evidence suggests that the manner in which one copes with stressful events plays an important role in the amount of stress experienced, and ultimately one’s psychological health. Most of this research indicates that direct coping is believed to be more beneficial for well-being than indirect coping (Thoits, 1995). Furthermore, there exists some empirical and clinical evidence that suggests that Latinos 54 employ more indirect coping strategies to manage stressful demands (Diaz-Guerrero, 1967; Weisz et al., 1984). However, there is no clear consensus in the literature regarding which c0ping strategies are most efficacious in reducing psychological distress (Aldwin & Revenson, 1987; Mattlin et al., 1990; Rodin & Salovey, 1989). Furthermore, some researchers have found that Latinos do use direct coping strategies such as relying on family, seeking support from fiiends, and spirituality (Abraido-Lanza, Guier, & Revenson, 1996) to cope with stressors such as acculturational stress (Mena, et al., 1987). Mental Health Psychologists have traditionally defined mental health as the absence of unhappiness or ill-being (e. g., depression, anxiety). This definition fails to acknowledge that mental health is more than just the absence of illness, disease, or dysfunction-it is the presence of psychological well-being, effective functioning in daily life, and the ability to deal with new situations (Berry, Kim, Minde, & Mok, 1987). Despite the fact that current research has demonstrated that psychological distress and well-being are correlated and are part of a two-dimensional latent construct which reflects a higher-order concept of mental health (Masse et al., 1998), most researchers continue to place little emphasis on the positive aspects of mental health status and continue to focus on the absence or presence of dysfunctional, psychopathological outcomes (Martin, 1989; Najera, 1990; Pavot & Diener, 1993). Distress. Studies concerned with Latinos’ mental health have most frequently used depressive symptomatology as an indicator of psychological distress (e.g., Cho et al.,1993; Golding & Bumam, 1990; Golding & Lipton, 1990; Roberts, 1992; Roberts, Roberts, & Chen, 1995; Roberts & Sobhan, 1992; Vega, Kolody, Valle, & Hough, 1986; Vernon, Roberts, & Lee, 1982). Most of these studies have addressed the extent to which 55 ethnicity increases or reduces the risk for depression. Thus, depressive symptoms for Latinos have been compared to those of Whites. The general findings indicate that both adolescents and adults tend to report a higher number of depressive symptoms than non Latina/o Whites (Golding & Bumam, 1990; Moscicki et al., 1989; Roberts, 1994; Roberts et al., 1995; Roberts & Sobhan, 1992; Vernon & Roberts, 1982). However, some studies show little or no differences in rates of depression between Latinos and Whites (Anthony & Petronis, 1991; Weissman, Bruce, Leaf, et al., 1991). Although the mental health of college students from various ethnic and racial groups has been identified as a priority concern by the US Public Health Service (Healthy People, 2000), relatively few researchers have examined minority college students’ mental health. Several researchers have found that exposure to a climate of prejudice on campus, racist experiences in college, and the lack of congruence between minority students (e.g., Latina/os) and the university are the most important factors impinging on the affective development or mental health of minority students attending predominantly White institutions (Fleming, 1984; Loo & Rolison, 1986; Suen, 1983; Tracey & Sedlacek, 1984, 1985, 1987; as cited in Cabrera & Nora, 1994). Studies that have examined levels or reports of depressive symptoms among Latino students (e.g., Constantine, Chen, Ceesay, 1997 ; Cuellar & Roberts, 1997; Rosenthal & Schreiner, 2000) indicate that (1) reported symptoms and prevalence rates of clinical depression are similar to reported symptoms and prevalence rates (2°/o-4%) found in community studies (Cuellar & Roberts, 1997; Rosenthal & Schreiner, 2000), (2) levels of depressive symptoms are higher for female and younger students than for males and older students (Rosenthal & Schreiner, 2000), (3) acculturation level is less influential than SES on 56 depression scores, (4) low SES increases the risk for depression (Cuellar & Roberts, 1997), and (5) depression is among one of the primary presenting concerns of Latina/os at a university counseling center (Constantine et al., 1997). Subjective Well-Being The last decade has seen a dramatic increase in research on the construct of subjective well-being (Diener, 1984; Diener & Larsen, 1993; Lyubomirsky & Lepper, 1999). Research has identified two broad aspects of subjective well-being: an affective component, often referred to as happiness (Diener & Emmons, 1984; Lyubomirsky & Lepper, 1999), and a cognitive component, which is referred to as life satisfaction (Andrews & Withey, 1976). Although the two components of subjective well-being are somewhat distinctive and can provide complementary information when assessed separately, they are not completely independent and have been found to be at least moderately correlated (Pavot & Diener, 1993). Subjective Happiness. Subjective happiness is the affective component of subjective well-being (Andrews & Withey, 1976). According to Lyubomirsky and Lepper (1999), subjective happiness is a global, subjective assessment of whether one is a happy or an unhappy person. The authors assert that this judgment is distinct from a simple sum of an individual’s recent levels of affect and/or satisfaction with life. For instance, one may appraise oneself as a very happy person, despite having only a somewhat happy life or vise versa (Lyubomirsky & Lepper, 1999). Previous research has found that several objective variables (e.g., demographic variables, life events) are correlated with happiness. However, some researchers (Diener, 1984; Lyubomirsky & Ross, 1997) have noted these correlations are lower than they ought to be. Researchers (e. g., Brickman, Coates, & Janoff-Bulman, 1978) have found that even extreme events (e.g., winning a 57 million dollars or becoming paralyzed) exert surprisingly weak effects on subjective well-being. Lyubomirsky and Lepper (1999) interpret these finding as evidence of that there is wide variation in individuals’ sources of their personal happiness. However, there is considerable agreement as to what happiness means and whether it has been achieved. Life satisfaction. Life satisfaction, the cognitive component of subjective well being (Andrews & Withey, 1976) has received little attention in the literature (Diener, Emmons, Larsen, &, Griffin, 1985). It is a subjective term that involves an evaluation or assessment of the quality of ones life on the basis of a personal, unique set of criteria (Shin & Johnson, 1978). More specifically, a comparison of one’s perceived life circumstances with a self-imposed standard or set of standards is presumably made, and to the degree that conditions match these standards, an individual reports high life satisfaction (Pavot & Diener, 1993). Life satisfaction is believed to be affected by time such that it is most influenced by events in the immediate past (Benjamin, 1994) but can also reflect a long-term perspective. In addition, it may be indirectly influenced by affect although it is not a direct measure of emotion (Diener, 1984). Nevertheless, a relationship between life satisfaction and emotional—well being exists since people may deny psychological distress but still be dissatisfied with life (Pavot & Diener, 1993). For the most part, psychological distress, rather than life satisfaction has been used as an outcome measure to assess college students’ mental health. The few studies that examined life satisfaction with Latina/o students found that their low levels of life satisfaction and happiness were significantly and consistently related to negative 58 perceptions about contemporary race relations (Brown, Wallace, & Williams, 2001) and acculturative stress (Rodriguez et al., 2000). . Integration of the Literature Review of the various constructs in the adapted stress-mental health model has revealed that Latina/o students appraise minority status and acculturative stress differently. Thus, not all Latina/o students are expected to experience psychological distress as a result of minority status stress and/or acculturative stress. The sparse literature with Latina/o college students has revealed that individual cultural characteristics, perceptions of social support, and coping strategies mediate stress outcomes. In particular, it has been shown that acculturation level and ethnic identity are related to stress and mental health. The literature has also shown that perceived formal and informal social support can buffer the effects of stress outcomes. In addition, direct and/or indirect coping strategies have been found to have varying effects on mental health outcomes. Finally, although most of the researchers that have assessed mental health status have concentrated on psychological distress, there is evidence that stress also has an effect on Latina/o students’ wellbeing. Review of the Literature Suppgrting the Hypothesized Paths in the Proppsed Model Although the literature has consistently indicated that individual cultural characteristics, perceived formal and informal support, and direct and indirect coping strategies affect mental health, the research has not clearly delineated the relationship amongst these constructs for Latina/o students attending a predominantly White university. Thus, the following section will review the literature that provides support for the hypothesized relationships among individual cultural characteristics (i.e., ethnic 59 identity, acculturation level), perceived social support, appraisal of sociocultural stress (i.e., acculturative stress, minority status stress), coping methods (i.e., direct and indirect) and mental health (i.e., distress, wellbeing). _Path A: The influence of cultural charafiteristics on perceived social support. Although social support has been fi'equently conceptualized as an environmental variable, an important consideration in understanding and appreciating its impact on mental health is the notion of individual differences, among support recipients. Researchers have suggested that some individual characteristics (i.e., agreeableness, extraversion, self- esteem, locus of control, generalized negative outlook, help-seeking tendencies, attitudes toward seeking and accepting help) may contribute to one’s perception of support (Lakey & Dickinson, 1994; Procidano, 1992; Procidano & Smith, 1997) and/or ability to extract needed social support (Cohen et al., 1986; Dunkel-Schetter & Bennett, 1990; Dunkel- Schetter et al., 1987; Taylor & Aspinwall, 1996). Although, few studies have examined the relationship between individual cultural characteristics and perceived social support, some findings suggest that there is a relationship between social support and acculturation among Latinos (Griffith & Villavicencio, 1985; Sabogal et al., 1987). Griffith and Villavicencio (1985) found that compared to less acculturated Latinos, more acculturated Latinos reported more reciprocal helping, contact with network members, larger support networks that extend beyond primary family to include fiiends and neighbors, and fewer symptoms of psychological distress. However, Sabogal et al. (1987) found that although referring or consulting with family was affected by the acculturation level of three Latino groups (e.g., Mexican, Cuban, and Central American), perceived family support remained high 60 despite acculturation level. In a longitudinal study of Latina/o students at predominantly White universities, Ethier and Deaux (1994) found that compared to students with low ethnic identity levels, students with high ethnic identity levels reported higher levels of social support from other Latina/o students, personnel (e.g., ethnic counselors) and the various supportive services made available by the universities for minority students. Bath B: The influence of cultural characteristics on appraisal of sociocultural m. The literature indicates that several individual characteristics (e.g., self-esteem, locus of control) differentially predict appraisal of stress (Fame et al., 1992), discrimination or minority status stress (Rodriguez et al., 2000; Shorey, Cowan, & Sullivan, 2002) and acculturative stress (Najera, 1990; Rodriguez et al., 2000). The appraisal of stress occurs in the context of an individual’s cultural values, beliefs, and experiences. Thus, individual cultural characteristics are believed to provide a context from which one appraises the threat of a given stressor (Cohen, 1992). Without this appreciation of the role of culture in the appraisal of stressors, we run the risk of misunderstanding the relationship between stress and mental health outcomes for Latinos (Miranda & Castro, 1985). The research has produced various conclusions regarding the relationship between individual cultural characteristics and appraisal of sociocultural stress. For example, some have reported that Latina/o college students at both lower levels of acculturation and ethnic identity are capable of experiencing sociocultural stress (Sanchez & Fernandez, 1993; Szapocznik et al., 1984; Roysircar-Sodowsky & Maestas, 2000). Others have found reports of greater stress among the more acculturated rather than among the less acculturated (Gilbert & Cervantes, 1986; Holck, Warren, Smith, & 61 Rochat, 1984; as cited by Neff & Hoppe, 1993). Some have even found that acculturation level was not associated with acculturative stress levels (Vazquez & Garcia- Vazquez, 1995). However, most have shown that Latina/o students with collectivistic orientations, lower levels of acculturation and/or lower stages of ethnic identity tend to report more sociocultural stress than students at higher levels of acculturation and/or higher stages of ethnic identity stage (Ethier & Deaux, 1994; Femandez-Barillas & Morrison, 1984; Montgomery, 1992; Negy & Woods, 1993; Quintana et al., 1991; Rodriguez, et al., 2000; Saldana, 1988; Saldana, 1990; Saldana, 1994; Sanchez & Fernandez, 1993; Shorey et al., 2002). Thus, the majority of these studies seem to support the acculturative stress model of Buriel, Calzada, and Vasquez (1982, as cited in Neff & Hoppe, 1993) which proposes that less—acculturated individuals, because of their values and behaviors are not adequately equipped to deal appropriately with the dominant culture. The result is sociocultural stress and hence, distress. Path C: The influence of perceived social supmrt on appraisal of sociocultural grass, It has been suggested by some that the perception of social support is one element in an individual’s appraisal of stress. That is, whether or not an individual perceives support from his or her social network depends upon the appraisal of threat that the person must respond to (Lazarus, 1966, 1981; Lazarus, Averill, & Opton, 1974). However, social support is also believed to influence a person’s appraisal of the stressfirlness of a situation (Cervantes & Castro, 1985; Cohen & McKay, 1985) and mediate the impact of stress on mental health (Lepore et al., 1991; Thoits, 1986). Researchers have found that Latina/o students who perceive support to be available are found to report less stress than students who perceive little social support to be available 62 (Solberg & Villarreal, 1997). More specifically, studies have shown that perceived family support is significantly related to Latina/o student’s experience of acculturative stress (Hovey & King, 1996). In addition, Latina/o students’ perceptions of formal support are related to feelings of sociocultural alienation (the outcome of holding values highly divergent from the majority) and reports of racial/ethnic tensions (Crouse, 1985; Hurtado, 1994; Loo & Rolison, 1986; Oliver et al., 1985; Ponterotto, 1990). Formal support factors that have been found to counter Latina/o students’ appraisal of sociocultural stressors include: (1) the presence of a residential, sociopolitical, academic community on campus that provides cultural support; (2) student support services that effectively serve minority students; (3) increased numbers of ethnic minority faculty to whom minority students can comfortable relate and (4) supportive and accessible faculty who impart a sense of academic and personal worth to students (Crouse, 1985; Loo & Rolison, 1986; Oliver et al., 1985). Path D: The influence of individual cultural characteristics on coping. Coping strategies have long been believed to be influenced by individual characteristics, cultural values, and norms (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984). Furthermore, individual characteristics are believed to exert their stress buffering effect on mental health via an effect on coping (Cohen & Edwards, 1989). Thus, coping strategies may be influenced by individual characteristics (e.g., self-esteem, optimism, internal locus of control) and reinforcement for particular ways of coping (Holahan & Moos, 1987; Aspinwall & Taylor, 1992; Taylor & Aspinwall, 1996). According to Slavin et al. (1991), individual cultural characteristics can be expected to have wide-ranging effects on coping strategies because ideas about the proper way to handle threatening or challenging events vary greatly from culture to 63 culture. For example cultures differ greatly in their beliefs about fate and the need to accept what fate decrees. Similarly, individuals within an ethnic group differ in feelings about their ethnic group. These feelings and beliefs strongly affect ethnic minority individuals’ appraisals of the usefulness of direct vs. indirect coping efforts. Furthermore, cultures both prescribe some coping behaviors and proscribe others (Slavin et al., 1991). Although few researchers have examined the impact of individual cultural characteristics on coping strategies, some have found that coping strategies vary as a function of acculturation status (Cervantes & Castro, 1985; Mena et al., 1987; Montgomery, 1992; Vazquez & Garcia-Vazquez, 1995). More specifically, bicultural and highly acculturated Mexican American university students have been found to use mostly direct coping approaches (Mena eat al., 1987; Vazquez & Garcia-Vazquez, 1995). However, Mexican-American students have also been found to utilize indirect methods of coping (Montgomery, 1992). Gomez and Fassinger (1994) found that bicultural Latina college students had a wider repertoire of coping behaviors than did Latinas primarily acculturated to either Latino or Anglo-American culture. Path E: The influence of perceived social support on c0ping. Lazarus and colleagues (1966; Lazarus et al., 1974) suggested that the perception of social support is one element in an individual’s subsequent coping with stress. Furthermore, coping strategies are dependent upon a person’s perception that support is available. Thus, perceived social support is seen as a dynamic resource that precedes, influences, and assists coping efforts (Cohen, 1992; Holahan et al., 1997; Lazarus & Folkman, 1984; Lepore et al., 1991; Thoits, 1986; 1995). Evidence for the link between perceived social support and both direct and indirect coping strategies comes from a series of studies by 64 Holahan and Moos and their colleagues (Billings & Moos, 1981; Cronkite & Moos, 1984; Holahan & Moos, 1986, 1987). In general, these studies indicate that individuals who perceive little social support are likely to engage in indirect coping strategies. In contrast, individuals who perceive that social support is available are more likely to engage in direct coping strategies. Just as perceived available social support has been linked to either direct or indirect coping strategies, in the context of stressful life situations, likewise, it has been hypothesized that the perceived availability of social support engenders either direct or indirect coping strategies for disadvantaged group members experiencing sociocultural stress (i.e., discrimination, Ruggiero, Taylor, & Lydon, 1997). However, few empirical studies (e. g., Ruggiero & Talyor, 1995, 1997, as cited in Ruggiero et al., 1997) have examined the relationship of perceived availability of social support to coping strategies with members of disadvantaged groups. These studies have found that women and ethnic minorities that reported experiencing discrimination tended to utilize indirect coping strategies when they perceived little to no available social support. However, those that reported experiencing discrimination but perceived that support was available were more likely to utilize direct coping strategies (Ruggiero et al., 1997). Although no studies have looked at the relationship between perceived social support and coping strategies for Latina/o students experiencing sociocultural stress, empirical findings indicate that when Latina/o students perceive support from family and school personnel (e. g, academic outreach programs, staff, and peers) it is helpful in their coping efforts and adjustment to college (Cooper, Jackson, & Azmitia, 1993; Cooper et al., 1998; Gloria & Rodriguez, 2000; Lopez, 1995; Rodriguez, 1994). A lack of such support has been identified as a 65 primary reason for Latina/o students’ inability to cope with stress (Fiske, 1988), poor adjustment, and high attrition rates (Alva, 1991; Lango, 1995). Path F: The influence of apgaisal of sociocultural stress on coping. According to Lazarus and Folkman (1984), stress occurs only when demands placed upon an individual exceed or tax the individual’s coping resources. Thus, stress always involves cognitive appraisals about the seriousness of the demand and the resources the individual has available to cope with that demand (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984). Thus, sociocultural stressors may not be perceived as stressful if an individual has the adaptive resources to cope with them and if they do not exceed the person’s ability to cope (Miller & Kaiser, 2001). Likewise, individuals’ appraisal of sociocultural stressors may affect coping strategies and ability to adapt (Berry & Kim, 1988; Williams & Berry, 1991). The appraisal process cues particular coping strategies that are then linked with an eventual adjustment outcome (Moos 1979, 1984, 1986). For example, indirect coping may occur when a stressor is perceived as stressful and individual and environmental resources are perceived as insufficient. Although few studies have found that members of diverse disadvantaged groups (e. g., women, ethnic minorities) utilize indirect coping strategies to cope with sociocultural stress (i.e., discrimination; Ruggiero & Taylor, 1995, 1997; as cited in Ruggiero et al., 1997), in general the perception of stressors in minority populations has been found to strongly predict both direct and indirect coping responses, supporting the supposition that both strategies are not mutually exclusive (Stein & Nyamathi, 1999). Thus, there is likely to be wide variability in how and with what effect people cope with sociocultural stressors. Moreover, sociocultural stress will be 66 detrimental to an individual’s mental health only if he or she is unable to cope with it successfully (Miller & Kaiser, 2001). Although coping reactions to sociocultural stress have been studied primarily at a sociological level (Ogbu, 1985), there is evidence in the psychological literature indicating that the appraisal of sociocultural stress appears to influence an individual’s ability to cope with it. For example, one’s appraisal of discrimination and injustice is believed to be an important determinant of coping strategies that are adopted by members of stigmatized groups (Schmader et al., 2001). Some individuals may indirectly cope with discrimination by internalizing negative stereotypes (Phenice & Griffore, 1994), attributing negative experiences to discrimination (Crocker & Major, 1989), and discounting feedback (Schmader et al., 2001). However, minorities can also engage in direct coping strategies toward perceptions of prejudice and discrimination by discussing it with the perpetrator, disproving stereotypes, using self affirmation (Phinney & Chavira, 1995), taking action to reduce the stress, and/or talking with others about the problem (Mena et al., 1987). Some have found that Latina/o college students who report experiencing acculturative stress use direct coping strategies to decrease the impact on college adjustment (Garcia—Vazquez, Vazquez, & Huang, 1998). In addition, some research indicates that Latinos and Caucasians do not differ in the types of coping styles used when faced with stressors. In fact both tend to use more direct than indirect coping (Mendoza, 1981). The variation in results is consistent with the growing body of research that suggests that the utilization of a given coping strategy, whether direct or indirect, is mediated by complex individual, environmental, and situational factors (Roth & Cohen, 1986; Folkman, 1984). 67 Path G: The influence of copig on mental health. To date, the relationship of coping styles to mental health is not clear. In the past, researchers have reported that is not stress per se but rather how people cope with it that affects one’s mental health. Although the directions of this relationship have been inconsistent, these studies have shown a strong association between coping and mental health (Billings & Moos, 1981; Folkman & Lazarus, 1986; Pearlin & Schooler, 1978). In some studies, indirect coping or high emotion-focused coping was found to be a psychological risk factor for adverse responses to stressful life circumstances (Cronkite & Moos, 1984; Holahan & Moos, 1986, 1987; Terry, 1994) and associated with a high number of psychological symptoms (Endler, Parker, and Butcher, 1993; Billing & Moos, 1981; Menaghan, 1982). Direct coping or high problem-focused coping on the other hand have been found to be associated with a low number of psychological symptoms or good adjustment to stressful events (Cronkite & Moos, 1984; Deisinger, Cassisi, & Whitaker, 1996; Holahan & Moos, 1986, 1987; Mena et al., 1987; Mitchell, Cronkite, & Moos, 1983; Stein & Nyamathi, 1999; Terry, 1994). However, some studies have revealed that people that used indirect coping strategies reported less psychological symptoms than people that used direct coping strategies (Asendorpf & Scherer, 1983; Linden, Paulhus, & Dobson, 1986; Mattlin et al., 1990). Finally, culturally specific coping patterns (e. g., culturally sanctioned beliefs, behaviors, practices, fatalism, religiosity) have been found to either buffer or place people at risk for specific negative outcomes (i.e., depression) (Neff & Hoppe, 1993; Vega et al., 1985). According to Miller and Kaiser (2001) the use of emotional regulation and expression and problem solving efforts are required before individuals dealing with sociocultural stress hit upon coping strategies that will promote 68 successful adaptation or good mental health outcomes. Thus, failure to express or regilate emotions resulting from sociocultural stress or utilize multiple problem-solving efforts could result in detrimental mental health outcomes. Current research examining the relationship between various coping styles and the mental health of college students has revealed a similar trend in results. For example, the use of direct coping styles has been found to be associated with higher levels of personal and emotional adjustment and fewer symptoms of depression and suicide risk (D’Zurilla, Chan, Nottingham, & Faccini, 1998; Essau &Trommsdorff, 1996; Leong, Bonz, & Zachar, 1997; Nezu, Nezu, Sarayderian, Kalmar, & Ronan, 1986). Deficits and lack of confidence in use of direct coping strategies or problem solving has been found to be related to higher levels of depression and hopelessness (Clum, & F ebbraro, 1994; Nezu & Ronan, 1988; Priester & Clum, 1993). Similarly, the use indirect coping has been found to be associated with increased depressive symptoms (D’ Zurilla et al., 1998; Essau & Trommsdorff, 1996) and lower levels of personal and emotional adjustment (Leong et al., 1997). These inconsistent findings may be due in part to different measures of psychological symptoms and coping. Furthermore, according to Carver and Scheier (1994), the literature on coping seems as a whole to be more informative about coping that interferes with good outcomes than about coping that facilitates good outcomes. According to the authors, this may be the result of posing research questions that focus on negative mental health outcomes (i.e., anxiety, depression) more frequently than posing questions that focus on positive outcomes (e.g., adaptation, psychosocial competence). 69 Chapter 2 RATIONALE Although a vast number of investigators from a wide range of disciplines have studied the relationship between stress and mental health, few have examined Latina/o undergraduate students’ psychological functioning when minority-status and/or acculturative stress are experienced. Nevertheless, several investigators have found that within the university environment, ethnic minority students, including Latinos, report experiencing cultural incompatibilities, discrimination, and prejudice (McCormack, 1995; Smedley et al., 1993). Furthermore, these stressful experiences have been found to ultimately impact their psychological well-being resulting in psychological distress and unsuccessful adaptation to the university (Morris, 1997; Najera, 1990; Quintana et al., 1991; Saldana, 1994; Shibazaki, 1999). The literature on stress and potential mediators has revealed that numerous individual, environmental, and process variables (e.g., self-esteem, goal directedness, locus of control, optimism, ethnic identity, acculturation, family support, social support, community involvement, and appraisal and coping processes; Aspinwall & Taylor, 1992; Constantine & Baron, 1997; Riggio et al., 1993; Shibazaki, 1999) mediate or moderate the relationship between stress and mental health in Latina/o undergraduate students. Furthermore, investigators have found that these variables have a complex relationship to each other and to mental health. However, the majority of these studies have isolated two or three of these variables at a time. Thus, they have limited explanatory power, indicating that the relationship between stress and mental health outcomes is relatively weak (Cohen et al., 1997). 70 Several researchers have developed complex models that emphasize the analysis of multiple levels in order to account for greater proportions of variance in outcomes and outline the dynamics of the stress-mental health relationship (Billings & Moos, 1982; Holahan et al., 1997; Miranda & Castro, 1985; Talyor & Aspinwall, 1996; Warheit, 1979). These researchers postulate that numerous internal and external factors, psychological processes, and the many interactions between them mediate the relationship between stressors and mental health. However, researchers have not attempted to test these or adaptations of these models with Latina/o undergraduate students. Instead, a few researchers have merely attempted to identify the relationship between one or two variables and mental health outcomes for Latina/o students. Furthermore, most of this research has taken place at universities where there is a significant concentration of Latinos (Aspinwall & Taylor, 1992; Morris, 1997; Padilla et al., 1986; Quinones, 1996; Rodriguez, et al., 2000; Saldana, 1994; Shibazaki, 1999; Solberg & Villarreal, 1997; Suarez et al., 1997). Clearly, there is a need to test dynamic stress-mental health models in order to understand the relationship between minority status, acculturative stress, and mental health in Latina/o students at predominately White, Midwestern, universities. To address this need, this study examined an adaptation of Taylor and Aspinwall’s (1996) model (see Figure 10) with Latina/o students at Michigan State University where Latina/o enrollment is low. The adapted model draws from I comprehensive approaches concerning the relations of personal, social, and external, resources to mental health (i.e., Billings & Moos, 1982a; Miranda & Castro, 1985; Leyva, 1990; Rodriguez et al., 2000; Saldana, 1994; Warheit, 1979). Thus, to address 71 £83 382 mamaoo 00min Ea 382 35885 28 80883.82 .3 839m mDrmmm Emma 3 and >10, respectively, indicating that excessive kurtosis and skewness were not present in the data set (Kline, 1998). The data also reflected a relatively normal distribution. Phase Two. In Phase 2, the psychometric properties of the measures were examined. Thus, the items of each measure were grouped according to each instrument’s hypothesized subscales. Observed correlations among the subscales of each measure 98 were examined. The internal consistency of each subscale was assessed by calculating coefficient alpha values. Alpha coefficients ranged from 0.51 on the Social Scale of the S.A.F.E. to 0.92 on the DSD26 (see Table 9). Because several of the scales had low internal consistency coefficients and were found to be highly correlated, confirmatory factor analyses (CFAs) using LISREL 8.3 (Joreskog & Sorbom, 1992) were conducted to determine if the collected data fit the reported factor structure for each measure. For measures that were reported to have more than one scale (e. g., ARSMA-II, MEIM, MSSS, S.A.F E Brief COPE, and PF-SOC), CFAs were also conducted to determine if a significant amount of the covariance among the subscales might not better be explained by one factor or a unidimensional factor solution. Floyd and Widaman (1995) indicate that unidimensional factor solutions may be appropriate for many psychological instruments because most psychological constructs are composed of multiple, correlated facets. To determine the degree of fit, absolute, relative and parsimonious fit indices were examined. To determine absolute fit or the overall fit of the model the chi-square (x2) goodness of fit index was examined. This index evaluates covariance among measured variables that are not accounted for by the models. Ifthe chi-square statistic is significant, there is statistical basis for rejecting the hypothesized factor structure of the instrument(s). The chi-square statistic is, however, dependent on sample size. Therefore, three additional indices were examined as recommended by Floyd and Widaman (1995) and Reise, Widaman, and Pugh (1993). These indices were the Goodness of Fit (GFI), the Adjusted Goodness of Fit (AGFI) (Joreskog & Sorbom, 1989) and the Root Mean Square Error Approximation (RMSEA; Steiger & Lind, 1980). The GFI and AGFI 99 indices range between zero and one and 0.90 is a suggested acceptable value. According to Browne and Cudeck (1993) a RMSEA value of 0.05 or less indicates a close fit to the data; 0.05-0.10 a moderate fit; and above 0.10 is a bad fit. The relative fit was assessed using the Comparative Fit Index (CPI; Bentler, 1990) and the Tucker-Lewis Index (TLI; Tucker & Lewis, 1973) or Non-Normed Fit Index (NNFI; Bentler & Bonett, 1980). The CPI and NNFI are indices relatively independent of sample size. CF I and NNFI values of 0.90 are considered as acceptable indicators of fit (Reise et a1, 1993). Relative parsimony of the null model (model with no common factors) and competing model (hypothesized factor structure of measure(s)) was determined by the Parsimonious Goodness of Fit Indices (PGFI and PNFI; Mulaik et al., 1989). These indices are relatively unbiased measures of fit that adjust for the degree of parsimony in each particular model. Higher values, ranging from zero to one, indicate better fit. Taken together, the indices of absolute, comparative, and parsimonious fit did not provide strong support for all measures except the SWLF and the SHS. CF As of unidimensional models of all measures excluding the SWLF and SHS did not provide strong indices of fit. Table 10 depicts these indices. Phase Three. The third wave of analyses explored the factor structure of measures that did not have acceptable fit. The items of each measure were submitted to Principal Axis Factor (PAF) analyses using SPSS 11.0 in order to determine the best factor structure of each measure for this sample. Because debate exists about the appropriate 100 t 2.8 .358“: 038.3% n mmm .28 e: as, 8:823 u 5.3 8223289 .3 28m 28 u 8de .28 93.3.... u 80mm. .23 2,5285 u .80me .25 258...... u Comm. .880 .885 "me08 wcaoo 825 n 3508. 22:52 u 3.72m :Eoom u .936 2.5323. n mmm:m n 55% 35350 5:88:85 u 832 ..mEoocoU .280 o_:£o=o:.o_:£m H «mm... $58.80 2:533. lmmmE ...occoaum biblebéonasm .38. 838th n mbmmn. ..mu=0_£-to&:m 58m 8385. u Emma SEE- e895 :38 3383. u <88 ..eosfiom 025m “952 3552623. 83: 255 u mam: 88.5.8 as. EEESG< H .5982 ..28m cocficomo £wc< u mfizmyz ..28M 5.35:0 58182 H 3.293. ..Ecomamm 05 so 323a ”EchoEooo 525.23. .802 .... .... ...: .... v... m... .... 2... ...~ 3.. 2.. 3.. 2.. N... 3.2 ...: __.~_ ...: .... .... .... .... ...: a ....N 2.2 M... ...: ...: ...: 2... 2... .... m... .... ...: .... ...: ...: 2... 3.2. 3... .... ...: ...: R... ...... :5: 3.... 3.. mm..- 2... 2... .... S... .... z... 2... 2... 2... 2... 2... .... ._.. .... .... z... .... .... z. 2.. E. a ma... 2... 2... M... 2... 2.. 3... 2... 2... .2. 2... .... 2... .... 2.. .... .... .... v... M... N... “:3. a .... Q. .... ...... 3... 2.. .... 2.. 2.. 2.. .... 2.. 2... z... 2... .... .... .... 3... .... ...... a 3.. 1.. .... .... 2.. 2.. 2.. .... .... 2.. .... 3... 3... 2... 2... 3... 3... .... .... Go... a ...... .... 2... 2.. _~.. .... ._.. .... .... .... a... 2... .... .... 8... 2... .... 3... do... a... 3... 3.. M... ...... .... M... V... .... .... .... .... 2.. .... .... .... .... .... 3o... 2.... .... 2.. 2.. v... .... 2.. .... 2.. .... M... 3... 2.. .... .... 2... N... 2.8. ...... N... 2... 2.. 3.. .... M... .... 3.. .... 3.. 2.. 2.. v... .... 2.. ...8. ...... 2.. 3.. .... 2.. .... 2.. .... .... z... .... .... .... .... 2.. a... a N... 3.. 2.. .... 2.. 2... Z... 2... .... .... ~... 2... 3.. 35 a 3.. .... m... .... 3... 2... 3... 2.. .... 2.. 3... .... 3.3 a... Z. .... N... 3... 3... : ... mm. 2.. m N. Z... 2.. _ .... a... 3.. .... 3... 3... 8... .M. .... .... S... .... an: a... .... 3... .... _ .... .... .... .... 2... 2.. as: a... .... .... .... .... .... .... 2... 3.. _ an: a 2.. _ ... ~_ .. .... .... ._ ... .... .3». ...... .... .... 2... ~.... .... 2... E... a... .... .2 2.. .... N... «.... .31.. ...... 3.. 2... M... 2:: a m... 2... .... 9...: 34. 2... .... E3: 94. an... as a 3:5 ...: :3. .83 02.. 03:89.. 3.8. 3.8. :3 $3 $3 :3 an: an: an: .2... E... «.... ”:3: 3:: £3: 3.: 3:3 883mg .0 mofiomnsm @2588 he. mafia—.80 BEDS—lam we. males—obloeoufi .0 03m... 101 applications of orthogonal (varimax option in SPSS) and oblique (promax option in SPSS) rotations when conducting PAF, both were performed. The resulting factor solutions differed significantly. Thus, Wood, Tataryn, and Gorsuch’s (1996) criteria for genuine factors were considered. These criteria are: large factor loadings, meaningful factors, and replicated factors identified in previous research. In addition, as recommended by Floyd and Widaman (1995), a factor was only considered if it consisted of at least three variables. The criteria for determining the number of factors to be extracted were based on eigenvalues greater than 1.0 (Guttman, 1954) accounting for 3% or more of the explained variance (Kachigan, 1991). Furthermore, as Floyd and Widaman (1995) recommend, solutions with factors that accounted for close to 50% of the variance of the measured variables were retained. Finally, the conceptual meaningfulness of the factors was considered in order to determine which factor solutions made the most sense based on theoretical or empirical grounds (Wood et al., 1996). The results of the PAF with orthogonal rotation met the above criteria best and are described below. The initial result of the PAP with orthogonal rotation was used to determine the number of forced factor solutions that needed to be conducted for each measure. Forced factor solutions were then inspected in terms of conceptual meaningfulness using the conventional criterion of factor loadings of 0.40 or higher. If an item had a factor loading of 0:40 or greater on more than one factor and the discrepancy between the item factor loadings was less than 0.30 the item was deleted from both factors. Once the best factor solutions were established, reliability analyses of each scale were performed. Additional items were removed from their perspective scales if internal consistency was improved 102 Sod vd... .59: .E .6 $2880 8285??“ u 75¢ 59: .E 3882 msoEoEmam n EZm 235 E 3.58-82 u £22 £85 .5 OEEEEoU n EU ”cosmEQoaa’x mo Bum 256m :32 Box n «Fame/E 235 .E .8 $2680 3833‘ n $0.4. 38?: am we $2680 n E0 ”888$ .6 82on "MW ”3:33-20 .1. Hula ”mum 295m M Z .202 8. mm. :2 3 8. a. 2... N 33 SN A32 5&3 a 523.8%; €85 52805623 mmm NM. 3. 3. Ma. S. a. S. m .53.: SN 52 ..a a 555 .685 aaoaaoaeaa 326 s. S. E. 2. :. S. 2. am .382 SN A82 ..3 a masom :39: 38355823 8-89 S. cm. 3. we. 2. 3 E. o: LENS SN £2 5.3m a. 8895 585 52808623 n58; mm. X. G. 3. S. S. 8. o: 3.?2 SN A32 5:5 a. 25.95 308:. 5228553 Emma on. E. 8. 3. a. S. G. O: 53.82 SN 25 .33: a. 039$ ”.085 52805625 $me 9.. mm. .2. 9.. mm. 3. E. m2 3%.: SN 385 5.280882: 08% S. 8. S. E. :. P. om. N: :33. SN A32 ..3 a bacom ”38:. 3.22086 9 want S. em. 2. 8. 2. S. 3. 8“ ...: .NS SN .085 3838563 mmoo firm 8. w... on 8. :. 3. E. mom .2»: SN 38 a: a. 03 .385 3.28086 3 Boo “0cm 8. mm. 3 S. S. 8. E. 2: Es: SN .086 522055: madam 8. 3. w». a. 2. E. cm. a: *8.an SN $2 ...a a 202 £88 32836 3 .mm.<.m 3. a. E. E. 2. S. S. E 33.32 SN 388 nagging $3 8. S. E. E. 2. E. 3. NZ .2 _ .Em SN :52 .283 M.25.: 38658.6 9 mmmz 8. 2. cm. 3. S. cm. 3. K $38 SN 38.: Baggage: Sam—2 S. 2. 3. a. 3. mm. 3. K Ana: SN A82 .322; .388 3283:. 9 Sam: 2. 9. M3. Nm. 2. mm. a. 2:. $32K SN 688 gage—nae: =-tom33m £5 5:3 couoflmumm macaqexm EEoEm mEquE>m Bambi 8.058% 023835 EcocoEm Bacomcom bacozcb Sow EoEfiSoQ 7258.3 onZcD 88m toqqsm uozooom 65033 basicb 9 toaazm wEBEE $8820 Bum toaasm boom $2255 tomasm coon 83803 595:: 358m toaqzm 383m wozooom mama—cu EmEo>< wanU o>mtomm< mamaoo 033$ toqum magnum wcioo o>to< mmobm 3:88885 EoEmm 3.52:0 8 26 233:2? 8330 3950 m0 _mm§&< comaEEtome mo _mm_8&< bmoifim mo 5:82qu gum—Bum mo aoEcmooom 8:23 0&2 8:85ch 953 mmm mug/m mama mama REG mmDmmm Ngmmm 52me gummm gmmm filmmmmm N2 «.2232 m 2 was: a 8.0 n 237.. 880mm 8%: £3» :o_§oomm< ”N 28m Rm. 3%.... owaawaH :mzmfiH 05 :2 0:0: mm waif: >2 oH<2myH< 3%... «e3. Emto 25235 H0 203 a: mEBEw 33 H 223 3:05 >2 NN<2mMH< 6820—. «2:0: 2%.- 3w. coon mm: >b::8 25c: >8 .23 88:8 >2 wH<2mm< $0.- vac. 25mg: 8 8:3 3 28?: >252 2 8:. H om<2mm< $0.- mmv. :mmto ..oESH. :o 2:235: Ho 8m 26: $55 >2 @2292 SN. Haw. $58 2535 8.08 >=E£ >2 v9.29? mgr cum. 28:65 £525: 8%5 85:3 5:.» 83088 H m<2mm< mmmr as. 23:: owmzmfix :3:an 8 3:85: >980 H 029?. SN. ms. 330:: owgmcfl :mmcmam >o.H:o H HH<2m~H< 03.. an. Emmfiam E 80on ..m8 of? H vH<2mMH< wow: 35. :2:an :H 8.8: ..m3 mficmo: >080 H 223?. wNN... «mu. :3:QO wEvHaoam >980 H 22mg? Her 3%. ommzw:£ zmmcmam 05 E 0:26 m_ @335 >2 53.29? as- E. Z. 88?: @525 3.3... H 35%. can- 2:. 5.68m 6:82 2292 m _ Geo: m: 8.0 n 2:2 286mm :ozficoto 0:33 ”H 28m a.<2mmoyHécm6€oE< @2682 :8 2QO mafia Egg—38¢. .NH 2an 108 The results of the subsequent forced factor solutions revealed that the two as opposed to the one forced factor solution was more conceptually meaningfiil. Item loadings for the first factor ranged from 0.421 to 0.795. This factor accounted for 46.66% of the explained variance and appeared to be a measure of recognition of ethnicity. All items of the Affirmation and Belonging and the Ethnic Behavior scales and one item from Ethnic Identity Achievement loaded on this factor. The one item from the Ethnic Identity Achievement and one item from Affirmation and Belonging were deleted because they also loaded on the second factor and the discrepancy between item loadings were approximately 0.13 and 0.26, respectively. Reliability analyses of this factor revealed that deleting one from the Ethnic Behaviors scale increased the alpha coefficient from 0.821 to 0.832. Item loadings for the second factor ranged from 0.516 to 0.717. This factor accounted for 8.04% of the explained variance and appeared to be a measure of one’s exploration of ethnicity. Four items from the Affirmation and Belonging scale loaded on this factor. However, one of the items was deleted because it loaded on both factors and the discrepancy between loadings was 0.01. Alpha reliability for this scale was acceptable (or = 0.72). The resulting two scales Recognition of Ethnicity and Exploration of Ethnicity are represented by MEIM] and MEIMZ in Table 13 and Figure 11. High scores on both of these scales indicate high ethnic identity. Appraisal of Sociocultural Stress Minority Student Stresses Scale (MSSS; Saldana, 1994 1. The M888 was used to assess stressful experiences and perceptions of the university relevant to ethnic minority status among Latina/o students. Saldana (1994) reported that the M888 is made up of three scales; Academic Concerns, Ethnic-Nonethnic Group Concerns, and Discrimination 109 82H H0088 080>0~H + 05:0 8085000 00.68:: 85200 0: 0080009 0080—00 80: ...... .Omdv 003 58000808 05 8.0 8.08 08 :05 088 :0 00802 2 000003 0000—00 80: ... 5mm. «mam. 08 8m .8008 8 00:3 80 8883009 0850 >8 .8 080m 0008 0 0>0n H MHszHSH 5000.05 0.80: 03. mwm. 988m 0850 >8 830 08000 .050 8 000:0“ 8050 020: H 68880—009 0850 >8 8000 088 800— 3 8008 5 SEE: :3. 0H m. 089030 080 £85805 >858 00 :03 .988 0850 :30 >8 8020 088 80 85 9. w8>b 085 80% 0>0.H H HSHHME :0. >2. 988 0850 >8 .8 >838 080 08:30 05 8000 088 E00. 8 w8>5 08: 80:8 80% 8: 0>0s >=008 H REES N _ 850 as. .o u 08:. 880g >8085m .8 85088me ”N 200m mwm. :50. 988 0850 >8 .8 802808 >588 00208 505 .8:on H088 8 8050~80w8 8 0380 80 H 3.5va ms... «80. 8:08 8050 80 008m :30 >8 8 00080 9 30: .8 08805 8 .08 8 8008 08802808 98% 0850 >8 505$ :03 >508 8.058058: H oSHHmHz 30. «HS. 988 0850 :30 >8 9 9088—030 0800 @850 0 0>0n H wSHHmHSH 508.05 080: mom. «cm. 8 8200 H 98% 05 .8 809808 0 80 H 005 >80: 80 H 2szqu 0mm. 03. 080852080000 0: H80 988 0850 >8 8 028 .8 8H 0 0>0s H HHSHHmSH Ham. an». 088.80 8 .0638 .008 880% 00 :08 .988 :30 >8 .8 0005008 H8380 8 000805.000 H 23:32 mom. 30. 82080—009 0850 8 H0880 >8 8090 team H00H H 3252 mom. man. 98% 0850 :30 >8 88039 80880050 mcobm 0 H00,H H mHEHmHHz m 8 H25: 3 3.0 u 082 8800a >.._085m ,8 :058w000yH ”H 200m Sam: 080002 5:03 08:5 -582 .m H 030% 110 Concerns. A CF A of these three subscales indicated a poor fit for the data [x2 (df 272, N=201) = 817.11, p < 0.001, GFI = 0.75, AGFI = 0.71, RMSEA = 0.10, CFI = 0.81, NNFI = 0.79, PNFI = 0.67, PGF I = 0.63]. A unidimensional model also resulted in poor fit [3;2 (df275, N=201) = 1241.48, 9 < 0.001, GFI = 0.67, AGFI = 0.61, RMSEA = 0.13, CFl = 0.72, NNFI = 0.70, PNFI = 0.61, PGFI = 0.57]. PAF analyses yielded two factors with eigenvalues greater than 1.0, each accounting for 3% or more of the explained variance. The two forced factor solution was found to be more conceptually meaningful than the one forced factor solution. Item loadings for the first factor ranged from 0.534 to 0.798. This factor accounted for 44.58% of the explained variance and appeared to be a measure of appraisal of discrimination. All items of the Discrimination Concerns scale and five items from the Academic Concerns scale loaded on this factor. Three items from the Academic Concerns Scale also loaded on the second factor and the discrepancy between item loadings ranged from 0.13 to O. 17, thus, they were deleted. The reliability coefficient for this scale was 0.90. Item loadings for the second factor ranged from 0.537 to 0.713. This factor accounted for 7.47% of the explained variance and appeared to be a measure of appraisal of campus culture. Four of the ten items from the Academic Concerns scale and eight of the Ethnic-Non Ethnic Group Concerns loaded on this factor. One item from the Academic Concerns scale was deleted because it loaded on both factors and the discrepancy between loadings was 0.14. Reliability analyses for this scale improved from 0.893 to 0.894 after removing one item from the Ethnic-Non Ethnic Concerns scale. Furthermore, deleting this item could be justified on theoretical grounds. The resulting two scales Appraisal of Discrimination and Appraisal of Campus Culture 111 are represented by MSSSl and MSSSZ in Table 14 and Figure 11. High scores on each of these scales are indicative of more experiences of minority status stress Social. Attitudinal. Familial and Environmental iSAFE.) Acculturation Stress Scale (Mena et al., 1987). The S.A.F.E. was used to assess stress arising from the process of acculturation in four broad areas: (1) pressure to assimilate and feeling impeded by cultural barriers, (2) stress that arises from separation from family, friends, and culture, (3) the quality of immediate interpersonal relationships, being sociable, and making friends, and (4) conflicts between personal and family values, expectations. The literature reports a four-factor solution (F uertes & Westbrook, 1996). These factors are: Environmental, Attitudinal, Social, and Familial. CFAs of the four-factor and one-factor solutions indicated a poor fit [Four-factor solution; 12 (df 183, N=201) = 538.00, p < 0.001, GFI = 0.80, AGFI = 0.74, RMSEA = 0.10, CFI = 0.81, NNFI = 0.78, PNFI = 0.64, PGFI = 0.63; One-factor solution; )8 (df 189, N=201) = 740.47, p < 0.001, GFI = 0.74, AGFI = 0.68, RMSEA = 0.12, CFI = 0.72, NNFI = 0.69, PNFI = 0.58, PGFI = 0.60]. PAF analyses yielded five factors with eigenvalues greater than 1.0 accounting for 3% or more of the explained variance. Only two factors met the criteria outlined above for appropriate scales. Subsequent forced factor solutions revealed that the two forced factor solution was more conceptually meaningful than the one factor solution. Item loadings for the first factor ranged from 0.509 to 0.799. This factor accounted for 33.88% of the explained variance and appeared to be a measure of alienation due to cultural barriers. Six items from the Environmental scale loaded on this factor. However, one item was deleted because it also loaded on the second factor and the discrepancy between loadings was 0.03. The reliability coefficient for this scale was 0.88. Item loadings for the second 112 0:20 8065000 00.68:: 880.0: m: 8:000: 00.0.00 80: ...... .cmdv 00.: 5:000:86 05 0:0 0200.: 0:0 :05 0:08 :0 00—000— : 08000: 00.0.0: 80: ... . .5. :o. 85.0 202 was“: 5%: «NS. 2.0. 95% 0850 >8 .00 080030 mo £000: 05 Sm toqqzm 0:0 800:8 w:_0_00_ 8803:: 02,—. vmmmz “0000—00 080: $0. wmm. 3:05:00 3:00 030—023 anew 0850 >8 mo 02080.: 80 00—08 :00309 08:80:23“ EmmmZ 8... EN. 0:28 €088 3003 a $8: an. mom. 20 :08 288 205 023 0o 20% B 06% 8 was: 2 3% 3m. mmm. 56:02:: 0:: H0 anew 0850 8: .00 809808 @080 30:03:0333080 .«0 0.004 3 392 can. woo. 0.95% 0850 8000.006 50.509 09:80:23” 2 mmmE 0%. 8m. 00535 as 050500 2:? 3:52 0:20 E 0559502 :30: 80. £0. 288 20.2 €88 0580 3: 9 0:30: 2302 03. mem. 980m 0850 b: .00 8800.00:— nmzofi “oz Emmi :0. Km. anew 0850 >8 8 800.0: 003$ m:_>_o>8 008:8 30m 332 80. com. 0080—0 »8 8 anew 0850 >8 .00 080an 30m $32 0:. $0 228 2088 0258-208 2 00004 N : 080: E 00 .o n 202 8203 0:830 0.6800 .00 0000830 ”N 200m 3%. .. 3m. anew 0850 N8 .00 080080 80.59 5300‘: .00 8080008008802 5mm: Ev. «new. 8803:: 0:5 .0 0.09.588 0:0 mo_w:< 50.509 0020:0320: 080,—. ommmZ an... 3%. 82 0805-52; 8 0:0 mo_m:< 0:0 Boa 08000-32 m800 9.2m 0850 »8 mo 0:09:08 w800m ommmz 68200 080: 3m. vnm. 8803:: $5 00 0003005 0:0 000:0: “£00m mmmmz Nwm. can. .00 0:000:09 00250 0.95% 0850 x8 .00 0300800080: 3 o: r. 00:3 005 080.005 3 mmmz NR. So. 95% 0850 .9: 00 080030 80¢ 00:08:80: 2800000 :00: 830090 b.300m\$:0cam o_w:< wmmmz NNM. mp. anew 0850 %8 .00 0300: .8.“ 000000: w:§00~ 8050 vmmmm—z owm. was. 20:0: 00 0035 0:03 ..w0v 8050 8 005—30 >8 :38? 9 8303 mmmmmz 02. $5. 36020 5 0o 0388 88.5 8 2003 0280 0:00 30mm: Em. 35. 389300005 80850 %8 mo 03 :03 80:00 0 0a 3 08 8:00:00 0300: o_w:< mmmmmz or. 02. 05000 0228800 0:00 20mm: N _ 85: a 3.0 n 202 8080a 5:088:85 me .0889? ; 200m mmmz 030m mmobm mSH—mwm utOcm—Z .V_ 255—. 113 factor ranged from 0.419 to 0.668. This factor accounted for 8.69% of the explained variance and appeared to be a measure of interpersonal stress. Items that loaded on this factor included: 4 from the Environmental, 4 from the Attitudinal, 1 from the Social, and 1 from the Familial scale. Three items from the Environmental scale were deleted because they loaded on both factors and the discrepancy between loadings ranged from 0.01 to 0.10. The reliability for this scale was 0.77. The resulting two scales, Alienation due Cultural Barriers and Interpersonal Stress are represented by SAFE] and SAFE2 in Table 15 and Figure 11. High scores on both scales indicate greater experience of acculturative stress. m The Brief COPE Inventory (Camer49fl. The Brief COPE was used to assess coping responses to minority status stress and/or acculturative stress. Carver (1997) reported 14 subscales each with two items. Lee and Liu (2001) reported two subscales COPE-Direct and COPE-Indirect. A CFA of the two-factor solution found by Lee and Liu (2001) indicated a poor fit for the data [x2 (df 208, N=201) = 748.45, p < 0.001, GFI = 0.75, AGFI = 0.69, RMSEA = 0.11, CFI = 0.60, NNFI = 0.56, PNFI = 0.48, PGFI = 0.61]. A unidimensional model did not improve fit [x2 (df 209, N=201) = 972.17, p < 0.001, GFI = 0.69, AGFI = 0.63, RMSEA = 0.14, CFI = 0.46, NNFI = 0.40, PNFI = 0.36, PGFI = 0.57]. The initial PAF yielded eight factors with eigenvalues greater than 1.0 that accounted for 3% or more of the explained variance. Only three of these factors met the criteria outlined above for appropriate scales. PAF analyses forcing factors ranging from one to three revealed that the three factor solution best reflected theoretical meaningfulness. Item loadings for the first factor ranged from 0.457 to 0.778. 114 05:0 8065000 00.08:: 8:200 00 000000: 020—00 80: .... 0de 003 8:80:80 05 0:0 8:00: 0:0 :05 088 :0 00002 H: 0000000 02200 82H .. «:3. 0n... 0:00—00 >8 .8 082000 000.008 H .H: 08 0000 0300 :00. 08000 055% «um... NNQ. 0:28:80 0: 08 0:08.08 0800: 00:3 08 80500 :H 092m «00m. 09.. 08000 :88 :05 080088 0: 80:80: 0:08 0>0: H 573% ”00.0.00 080: 03.. mum. 080: :0 H00: :.:00 H meHnH030 0>08 0: 08 80>» :0: 0.000 >=80.: >2 HNmHnH8 .8 08000 0300 :00 :0 8000 8on 0x08 8050 :05» 0308580000 .00.: H 0mm8 5;» 005 05 m800000H H HmHhH=00: H 30: 00:05.: >8 0: 8.0800 0: 0:0: 0: :H vaHnH05 .: 00 08 :00: 0:0 @008 0850 :0 08:00 >8 8000 00900085 00: 08000 >002 vmnzm “080.00 080: 2.0.. m3. 850:8: 0 8 38850 >8 :05 H008 00850800 H .02 0 8,: 8200— :H mmm8 .8 000000m wmnzm mNH. awn. 880000 800 08 00:0 0: >5 >H0>500 08000 :05 H00: 0000 H $505 :3. 00:. 08 :880 0: 0000800 0:0 05> 08000 >0 008:3 H00: 0050 H Nmm:m 0.0 _0=_80nH 8800500. 300m .2 030.0 115 This factor accounted for 20.81% of the explained variance and appeared to be a measure of active coping. Eight items from the COPE-Direct scale loaded on this factor. Reliability analyses produced an alpha coefficient from 0.82. Item loadings for the second factor ranged from 0.768 to 0.821. This factor accounted for 12.89% of the explained variance and appeared to be a measure of seeking support. Two items from the COPE-Direct scale and two items deleted from the COPE-Indirect scale by Lee and Liu (2001) loaded on this factor. The alpha coefficient for this scale was 0.88. Item loadings for the third factor ranged from 0.434 to 0.581. The third factor accounted for 7.14% of the explained variance and appeared to be a measure of passive coping. Seven items from the COPE-Indirect scale and two items deleted from the COPE-Indirect scale by Lee and Liu (2001) loaded on this factor. Reliability analyses revealed that the reliability increased from 0.75 to 0.76 when one item from the COPE-Indirect scale was deleted. The resulting three scales, Active Coping, Seeking Support, and Passive Coping are represented by BCOPEI, BCOPEZ, and BCOPE3, respectively (see Table 16 and Figure 11). High scores on these scales indicate high utilization of direct coping strategies (e. g., Active Coping, Seeking Support) and high utilization of indirect coping strategies (e. g., Passive Coping). Problem-Focused Style of Coping (PF-SOC; Heppner et $1995). The PF-SOC was used to assess stable dispositional coping styles that tap general coping strategies. The literature reported three subscales Reflective Style, Suppressive Style, and Reactive Style. A CFA of the three-factor solution indicated a poor fit to the data [Three Factor; )8 (df132, N=201) = 439.91, p < 0.001, GFI = 0.80, AGFI = 0.75, RMSEA = 0.11, CFI = 0.76, NNFI = 0.72, PNFI = 0.60, PGFI = 0.62] A CFA of a one-factor solution produced 116 .0020 800000000 0908:: 0000.00 00 0000000 00000 80: ...... 3.090. mnv. one. 09.. 0mm. 0mm. 00m. 00m. Ham. m wmor wNor omo.- mmor m 35.- 00H. 08. mmo. 35.. o: .. 000. N8. m ch. 03. 03.- N09. pot mNo. ooo. HNo. GONr N 0:00.000 new. 000. Ht. HNm. N 05000.0 mmN. coo. 0mm. HMN. moN. 0HN. woo. 000. N 88000 w080000 :0 $083.0 .w00000:0>00 000000: .>.H. 8:00:05» .8068 0: w80w 00 0000 .002 0 00000 085 0: 8000800 00 H :0000 .00.: 0H00>8 0008 0: 0w0:0 :050 :0 8000.0 0000 H 0000 0: 88000 05 00 02w H 8:89... 09: 0 0.0 320.: 2 3.02 : 0H00>8 080000 H 0 @085 00m 08 030 0: 08:0 :050 :0 600000 000 H :80: 0.000 0.05.. 0H00>8 0: >00 H 0 00.5 H000 0: w00>:: 00 0.08 H 00000000 :05 0m000 :00 0H00>8 0803 H a H mHnHOUmH ”0000—00 080: 0m000m 0 H mmOUm ammoom m H mmOUm H HmHnHOUm mmHOOUmH emaoom cNmHnHOUmH A053: 00 03 u 20:0 8800 000000 20000 :050 800 00300 0:0 000 :00 H 0000800 800 8000080000 000 00,088 0m H 0:050 800 000000 0800080 0w H 00 0: :00: :0000 20000 :050 800 000 :0 000,00 00w 0: >0 H m 200m 9 H mHAHOUmH m H mEOUmH nmnHOUmH mNmmoom 00.3: 3 00.0 u 0008 ::0000m w80H00m 00000000 000 0 :05 000.: 05 .:0 >88: 05 80000 H 0 003 03H 0: 800H H 030000 0:08 8000 0 0008 0: .080 820.00 0 8 0 000 0: >0 H 00 :5 0000000 05 0:000 8050800 m0000 00 000.00 >8 300000000 H 8800000 0: :00? 8 000m 8050800 :00 002 H :0000 0000000 05 0008 0: >5 0: 00000 000: H 000: 0: 000:0 :00? 8000 0:00 085 H 00 0: 0003 8000 $000.00 0 00B 00 0800 8 >0 H N 003m ONmHnHOUmH 0Nm000m N H mmoom NmHOOUmH 0 H mnHOUm hmnHOum mNmHnHOUmH 0H mmoum 00.3: 00 ~00 u 2:2 m0H0ou 0>00< H 200m 252.: 0.50 0000 2F .0. 200.0 117 poorer fit indices [ )6 (df 135, N=201) = 5148.74, 2 < 0.001, GFI = 0.54, AGFI =04], RMSEA = 0.23, CFI = 0.40, NNFI = 0.32, PNFI = 0.33, PGFI = 0.43]. The initial PAF yielded four factors with eigenvalues greater than 1.0 each accounting for 3% or more of the explained variance. Subsequent forced factor solutions revealed that the two forced factor solution was the most theoretically meaningful. Item loadings for the first factor ranged from 0.598 to 0.754. This factor accounted for 22.69% of the explained variance and appeared to be a measure of assertive coping. All items of the Reflective Style scale loaded on this factor. The reliability of this scale was found to be 0.85. Item loadings for the second factor ranged from 0.415 to 0.678. This factor accounted for 22.04% of the explained variance and appeared to be a measure of avoidant coping. All items from the Suppressive Scale and five items from the Reactive scale loaded on this factor. The reliability of this scale was 0.72. The resulting two scales Assertive Coping and Avoidant Coping are represented by PF -SOC1 and PF-SOC2 in Table 17 and Figure 11. High scores on PF-SOCI indicated high utilization of direct coping strategies and high scores on PF-SOCZ represented high utilization of indirect coping strategies. Perceived Social Support Perceived Social Support from Fafimily (PSS-FA; Procidano & Heller, 1983). The PSS-F A was used to measure the extent to which needs for support is met by family. Factor analyses have revealed that the PSS-F A is composed of a single factor. A CF A of the one-factor solution revealed poor fit [x2 (df I70, N=201) = 1268.68, 9 < 0.001, GFI = 0.61, AGFI = 0.52, RMSEA = 0.21, CFI = 0.64, NNFI = 0.61, PNFI = 0.54, PGFI = 0.50]. Initial PAF analyses yielded three factors with eigenvalues greater than 1.0 accounted for 61.99% of the explained variance. Only two of these factors met the 118 2... 08.. £288 2: 58“ @255 :26 2o: _ ”Comma we... Hm H. 6088 8a 286288 >8 .: 8m 9 8083 of £5 wciooso 82:3 mmczofl 28 $388 P8868 82:08“ 3288 H mHUOmmnH 3v. 2N- =m 8 mEoan 2: co x53 8a mcmou a: 03m 3% H 335mg 8 HooH H mUOmanH HHm. $0.- mEoan 05 Soon 05:3 8 #85 H H23 88 3.88 8: :8 H HUOmmnH mam. moH .- $8353 888 >8 :9: mEOEEa 2: co $388888 08: 2356 a 028 H oHUOmmnH Nam. «NH .- 0803 mEoan mow—«8 .323 $208: 09 8m H m 6093 HHe. 0H m. 82: .Ho 2.8g 08% 053;an.03 can m82noa of Bonn magi: 88:80qu 8m H hUOthAH n3. mom. 483 808 088 ow 8 woo: H 08 £8 :oEB £80308 2: Sons $88: .00H 2 0:888 H wUOmHHnH m5. wmo. mEOEEQ 80:3 @838 .Ho 33 2: 8 How amazoom 20 >2 QUOmanH as. moor mEoan 2: co mason Ho 8358 8:338 98 8.55 ©9208: wEoc 08: >88 85% H 200th N H 98% 2va .o u 9&2 828m mcaou 8mEo>< “N 28m Hmo. mam. WEBDEQ 2: :0 x53 can @802 2 $815 a 53» 20:2 8 EWH 300th $0.. an. x53 8: meow 8888 Eu >8 0me 8 m8oan 2.: 9:28 88 888 88:83 0%: H :UOmanH owe... «me. mEOBEQ 05 028 was @802 08 36: 2023 82880 >8 H0 838 05 @3802 H vUOmmm 2 o. are. $3 2: E mEoBoa 8:86 328 H 85 $33 Sosa 8:: H mUOmanH .20.- 93. mm: >2: 8&3 mEoan SH 2308 88 083285 9 08 3388 £033 63% :85 H NHUOmanH mg. mEoBoa 30 of 3 80:28 0358a some Ho 8053388 888 man: 98 8.83.55 05 6288 H @0095 08... vmb. 8: >05 8&3 mEoan SH 2808 was 238:5 9 08 3395 £023 68% x85 H 20093 N 8 85: t 3.0 n at? 828m mcaou o>_tomm< ”H 28m UOmJHAH 8 come 2 Hm vomsoom-80_noi .2 2an 119 criteria outlined above for appropriate scales. PAF analyses forcing both one and two factors revealed that the two factor solution best reflected theoretical meaningfulness. Item loadings for the first factor ranged from 0.579 to 0.825. This factor consisted of 10 items accounting for 40.02% of the explained variance and appeared to be a measure of received family support. Reliability analyses of this scale produced an alpha coefficient of 0.9]. Item loadings for the second factor ranged from 0.641 to 0.810. This factor contained 5 items accounting for 6.50% of the explained variance and appeared to be a measure of family intimacy. The alpha coeflicient for this scale was 0.84. The resulting two scales, Received Family Support and Family Intimacy are represented by PSSFA] and PSSF 2, respectively (see Table I8 and Figure 11). High scores on these scales indicate high perceptions of support from family and high perception of family closeness. Perceived Social Support from FriendsjPSS-FR; Procidano & Heller, 1983). The PSS-FR was used to measure the extent to which needs for support is met by college peers. Factor analyses have revealed that the PS S-FR is composed of a single factor. A CFA of the one-factor solution revealed poor fit [3(2 (df 170, N=201) = 1073.44, 9 = 0.001, GFI = 0.65, AGFI = 0.57, RMSEA = 0.16, CFI = 0.65, NNFI = 0.61, PNFI = 0.54, PGFI = 0.53]. Initial PAF analyses yielded three factors with eigenvalues greater than 1.0, each accounting for 3% or more of the explained variance. PAF analyses forcing factors ranging fi'om one to three revealed that the three factor solution best reflected theoretical meaningfiilness. Item loadings for the first factor ranged fiom 0.539 to 0.765. This factor accounted for 39.49% of the explained variance and appeared to be a measure of received peer support. Four items were deleted because they also loaded on the second factor and the discrepancy between loadings ranged fi’om 0.13 to 0.20. Reliability 120 80: 00800 080.6m t .N a. H 8908 so U082 80: 8:. ...... .Omdv 83 5:30.86 08 88 8908 0:0 .85 088 so 382 a 00:80: 0000—00 82H .. H3. :5. 0388008008: 08 00088 x .>=88.H >8 .8 83808 8 08.88 H 8055 +0H6 is 8 +8882 0%. Hmo. 038888008: 805 00088 8 85 802 05 8m H .08 8 80020 08 0:3 >=8£ >8 .8 809808 05 8 03800 H 8055 835mm awn. 05. 83808 >=88H :23 8282888 0.0303 8050 8 0020 8 8 85 >=80H >8 00 809808 8 FEB 250828—08 0 08: H.820 H +oH288H 805 3 80020 08 0308 80:8 0002 +2.,Hmmm m 8 $50: 38 .o n 282 8800a >88ch >=80HH ”m 080m «Lev. $.00. 08 SH $85 0088 go mWEE ow 8 Bo: 828 802 coow 8w >__80.H >8 .3 809802 mH» Soon @880; m>o.H:0 >=8£ >H>H m883 w8H00H 80:83 .8520 m8_00.H 8% 0003 H .H_ 8 ow 2:00 H >HH88H >8 .8 802808 0 fl 0805. gamma 05.- «an. $008008 >8 Ho >88 08% >=8£ >8 Ho 800802 o28& >8 88H mwfifi £0 8 26: 828 802 voom 8w H «gamma 000. «we. >:88,H >8 .3 809808 Ho 8388 8 :83 8:80:20. @885 @020 8 08: H vH8 8 03:88 mm >=88H >H>H HH=80H >2 oHHH88H >8 co >H08 H w=88H >2 H=8fl >8 .8 809802 mH=80nH 80>_000~H ”H 080m :58 620 to am 880 838008 .2 033. 121 analyses of this scale produced an alpha coefficient of 0.89. Item loadings for the second factor ranged from 0.638 to 0.799. This factor contained 4 items accounting for 13.95% of the explained variance and appeared to be a measure of support provided to friends. The alpha coefficient for this scale was 0.87. Item loadings for the third factor ranged from 0.565 to 0.756. This factor contained 5 items accounting for 6.95% of the explained variance and appeared to be a measure of peer closeness. The alpha coefficient for this scale was 0.82. The resulting three scales, Received Peer Support, Providing Peer Support, and Peer Closeness are represented by PSSFR], PSSFR2, and PSSFR3 respectively (see Table 19 and Figure 11). High scores on PSSFR] and PSSFR2 indicate high perceptions of receiving and providing support to peers. High scores on PSSFR3 indicate high perceptions of peer closeness. Perceived Social Support from University Personnel (PSS-UIQ. The PSS-UP was used to measure the extent to which needs for support are met by university personnel. A CFA of a one-factor solution revealed poor fit [12 (df 170, N=201) = 822.71, p = 0.001, GFI = 0.71, AGFI = 0.64, RMSEA = 0.14, CFI = 0.68, NNFI = 0.64, PNFI = 0.56, PGFI = 0.57]. Initial PAF analyses yielded four factors with eigenvalues greater than 1.0, each accounting for 3% or more of the explained variance. PAF analyses forcing factors ranging from one to four revealed that the three factor solution best reflected theoretical meaningfulness. Item loadings for the first factor ranged fiom 0.503 to 0.838. This factor accounted for 34.62% of the explained variance and appeared to be a measure of providing support to university personnel. One item was deleted because it also loaded on the second factor and the discrepancy between loadings was 0.03. Reliability analyses of this scale produced an alpha coefficient of 0.87. Item loadings for the second factor 122 80: 0082 0208.: + .onv 83 28:88:: 2: :8: 88:: 0:0 :2: 208 :0 320:0: : 08:00: 888: 80: . mom. ewe. ewc. an. emu. m Ego. mmo. moo. wno. m voor moo. vac. N2. :0. mNo. £9. £0. 2N0. 2N0. Noe. m :3. m2. mvor mNo. 80.. N 208:: «no. NS. n 2.. :3. N 208: m 8:. m3. 3%. mac. mmm. mNN. voN. :N. com. 9:. moo. N 208:: moor :mo. NNo. moo. So. chm. ocm. mmN. mmm. comm. cvmm. «3e. ..mvo. Nam. :3. NN:. 9N2. «Nb. mmfi mob. 0388:8008 08 82:8 : daew 82:28 .8 8:808 8 03800 _ 8:3 bammm: 0388:8008 805 002:8 : 86 :02 2: 8w _ .08 0: 208.0 2: 0:3 82w 80: 28 .8 20:808 05 8 08.800 _ :25? 82.2mm: 820.2%: :88 203 200: 28 :23 _ 895mm: 20:808 :88 80: :85 2:380:88: M0800: 8:8 m: 208 m: m: 85 :88 80: 28 .8 8:808 : :85 8:80:22 : 02:: #:0: _ 835mm: 8: _ :2: 200: :05 0: 88—0 2: 0800: 8:8 802 .953: 22.2 a :3 u 2:? 20800—0 80: m 080m 988088800 8: 80 08 0:000 :88 80: 28 .8 20:82.: Smmmm: 003:: :00: 8 m80_:2: 0>:: 20:: 8:3 08 8 0800 82w 80: 28 .8 20:808 8:200 53mm: 0.82:2: 0200 82: 888: 8 :00m 82 8:: 82 200: 28 8:. 0.8:: Emu—mm: 20:98 88880 8.: 08 0: 0800 :88 80: 28 .8 20:82.: Nammm: $52 3 :8 u 2:: 20:86 80: 8:28.: N 080m 08:: _ 8:3 80:: 82:0: 2080 200: 22 22mm: 88:: 80:: mg: 03 8:3 80:: :08 28> 2: H 8: 200: 22 222mm: 2: 8: 88:: 02:8 8 88:: 0: 0: 30: 80:: 0:02 :00m 8m :88 80: 28 .8 20:82.: mammm: :88 80: 28 8 20:808 .8 8:88 : :23 8:80:22 8228 :00: : 0>::_ Edmmm: 608:0: m80: 88. z 80:: 2:8: 8:00: 80:3» :30: 8:00.: 88. 08.5 _ .2 0: 0w 2:00 _ 82w 80: 28 .8 8:808 : 2 22:. :Mmmm: 2000: 8:020: 28 0: 03:80: 2: 200: 22 Zammm: 20228 28 .8 28:8 08:: 80% 80: 28 .8 20:82): cammm: 2:032: 020: 08 88—0: 8 :00w 2: ::2w 80: 28 .8 20:80.2 mammm: 88880 80.: 200: 28 :0 2.2 _ mammm: :00: _ 20:98 888 0:: 08 02m 200: 22 35mm: 200: 28 80:: 88:: 0088 8 88:: 0: 0: 30: 80:: 0:02 80w 8w _ Nammm: 22.2 t 98 u 932 20:96 80: 830002 _ 080m 123 ranged from 0.488 to 0.717. This factor accounted for 10.82% of the explained variance and appeared to be a measure of received support from university personnel. One item also loaded on factor one and the discrepancy between loadings was 0.13. This item was deleted. The alpha coefficient for this scale was 0.85. Item loadings for the third factor ranged from 0.454 to 0.645. This factor accounted for 8.77% of the explained variance and appeared to be a measure of students’ relationship to university personnel. Reliability analyses of this scale indicated that the alpha coefficient would increase from 0.65 to 0.66 if one item was deleted. The resulting three scales, Providing University Personnel Support, Received University Personnel Support, and Relationship to University Personnel are represented by PS SUP], PSSUP2, and PSSUP3 respectively (see Table 20 and Figure 11). High scores on PSSUPI and PSSUP2 indicate high perceptions of provided and received support from university personnel. High scores on PS SUP3 indicate high closeness to university personnel. Mental Health The DSM ScaLe for Depression-26 (DSD-26; Roberts efl.4 992,. The DSD-26 was used to assess symptoms of depression. A CFA of this one factor depression measure was poor [x2 (df 299, N=201) = 1006.85, 9 < 0.001, GFI = 0.72, AGFI = 0.67, RMSEA = 0.11, CFI = 0.76, NNFI = 0.74, PNFI = 0.62, PGFI = 0.61]. The initial PAF yielded six factors with eigenvalues greater than 1.0 that accounted for 3% or more of the explained variance. Only three of these factors met the criteria outlined above for appropriate scales. PAF analyses forcing factors ranging from one to three revealed that the three factor solution best reflected theoretical meaningfiJlness. Item loadings for the first factor ranged from 0.440 to 0.724. This factor accounted for 37.53% of the 124 :82 80.2.2000 832:8. : 08:00: 88.0: 80:. ...... 80:. :28: 020.20.: » .omov 83 20:80:02: 0:: :2: 88:: 0:0 8:: 0:08 :0 :0::0. : 08:00: 88.0: 80:... .8822: 2:20:28: ::3 82828.2 .. 8m... o no. owor m.0.:00: 8:8 8 08.0 m: m. 8:: .0888: 2:20:28 0:: .8 8:808 : ::3 8:80.88: : 0.8: :.:0: . 875mm: ”:0:0.0: 2:0:— vbm. mmo. :N.. 0.8228008: 8:: 00088 : 8:: :0:. 0:: 8w . .08 0: 208.0 2: 0:3 .8820: 2:20:28: 0:: .8 20:808 8 08.800 . :25? +85%: 8N0. 03.- 8:. 220:: :28 23. 288:2 282:3 2:: ::3 : .282: new. 98.. moo. 0.820.808: 08 8088 : .8888: 2:20:28: 0:: .8 20:808 8 02.800 . 8:3 +:.:Dmm: m N : .82 m: 8.: u 9:8 20:0: ... .2888: 2:20:83 0: 8:80.88: m 0.:0m .oo.- «vwm. om... 88:: 80:: ::.:: 03 8:3 80:: :0:0 28> 2.: . :8 88020: 2:22:83 oEDmm: 820.0: :80:— :3. «we. SN. 2888. 28 .8 28 08:0. .8820: 2:288: 058 20:80.). ::Dmm: woo... :5. mwm. 800: .8020: 28 0: 02:80: 0:: .8820: 2:20:83 ..gmm: .oo. m8. N. N. :00: . 20:80 8:08 0:: 08 0.28 .8820: 2:20:83 33mm: :3. 30. 3:. .8: : 8:3 :82 2:8: 20.8.. 2885: 2822: 8:2: ooo. no». moo. 8:820: 2:288: 80:: 88:: 0088 8 88:: 0: c: 30: 80:: 80:. :00w :8 . Ngmm: m N : .82: 8 2.: u :88 208: ”. 20:::m .8820: 2:20.283 83003. N 0.:0m wmo. m8. «NNm. 08 :0: 88:: 0088 8 88:: 0: 0: 30: 80:: 80:. :00w 8m .8820: 2:20.28: 0:: .8 20:80.). 2:53: 608.0: 2:0:— 88. : 80:: 2:8,: 8:00.: m . o. NR. mom. 808.3 :30: 8:00.: 8:2 0:03 . .2 0: 0m :.:00 . .8820: 2:228: 0:: .8 8:808 : m. 08:... ::Dmm: .oo. mvm. moo. m80.:0:: 0>.0m 82.: 88.0: 8 :00w 8.. 8:: .00.: .8820: 2:20:38: 0:: 8:: 0.8:: . w 33mm: o.o. ...m. 5:. 20:80 88880 8.: .08020: 2:20.28: :0 2.2 . wgmm: Nmo. m2. and. 2888. 28 .8 2:8 08:: .28020: 2:20.28: 8:200 ::Dmm: m:o.- mom. mg. .8820: 2:288: 0:: .8 20:808 .8 8:88 : ::3 8:80:88: 8280 :00: : 08:. 35mm: .oo. mN.. :8. 20:8: 88.880 8.: 08 0: 0800 .8820: 2:20.28: 0:: .8 20:80.). N.:Dmm: NNo. 8.. an». :.:808::800 8.: 80 08 0.00: .8820: 2:20:28: 0:: .8 20:80.). ...:Dmm: m N : .82 o 28 u :82 20:0: n. 20:::m .8820: 2:20>8D 88:20:: . 0.:0m . :D..mm: .08020: :20>.:D 88.: ::0 :m 8.00m :0>.00:0: .oN 0.8... 125 explained variance and appeared to be a measure of emotional depressive symptoms. Reliability analyses of this scale produced an alpha coefficient of 0.88. Item loadings for the second factor ranged from 0.449 to 0.799. The second factor accounted for 8.55% of the explained variance and appeared to be a measure of physical symptoms related to depression. Two items that also loaded on factor 1 were deleted. The discrepancy between factor loadings for these two items was 0.09 and 0.12. The alpha coefficient for this scale was 0.84. Item loadings for the third factor ranged from 0.539 to 0.840. This factor accounted for 4.87% of the explained variance and appeared to be a measure of suicidal symptoms. One item also loaded on factor 1 and was deleted. The discrepancy between factor loadings for this item was 0.08. Reliability analyses for this scale produced an alpha coefficient of 0.82. The resulting three scales, Emotional Depressive Symptoms, Physical Depressive Symptoms, and Suicidal Symptoms are represented by DSD], DSD2, and DSD3, respectively (see Table 2] and Figure 11). High scores on these scales indicate high level of emotional, physical, and suicidal depressive symptoms. Satisfaction with Life Scale (SWLS; Diener et al., 198;. The SWLS was used to assess life satisfaction. A CFA of this one-factor measure of life satisfaction was good [x2 (df 5, N=201) = 14.48, p < 0.001, GFI = 0.97, AGFI = 0.92, RMSEA = 0.10, CFI = 0.98, NNFI = 0.97, PNFI = 0.49, PGFI = 0.32]. This factor structure was accepted given similar reported results. For the current sample, the alpha reliability was 0.89. This scale is represented by SWLS in Figure 11 and high scores indicate high levels of life satisfaction. 126 32.350 2an oom. CNN. moo. ovo. coo. 02. mm _. m 03. EN. 3:. gm. o2. v3. v3. vvm. :N. me. a3. ace. Nnm. 8m. 3%. 23. am». N £88m ONN. vam. wmfi. mum. vwm. mw_. mum. mum. hwm. 5m. N 3323 EM. on»: omm. owm. omo. com. mom. :3. «3. a3. Sm. Nam. nan. mmm. new. ewe. 9N5. _ 08: 2: mo .88 958m :8 ma. 8 v22. 20% can mama—KEN E 33885 “.583 33. 50% 5:3 8:5 82: 835 mcofiooc 838 8 8 mwfifi Sosa 2:8 50% a: BEE 8 so» Em SEE 33 z 88: 82: 803 33: 52: 32 § a 258m 358 8 vex—S so» 3a: :25: 3 >22? mm 8 >28? 3 x55 8 382588 9 2825 coup so» 0%! 33m: :2: 808 8_ a 82m so» 262 3258 on 9 tote m5 a £3 z 5.: cm .33: 52.: 3.55 $2 :028 3: sex BE “5 :9» 26m mast?“ mo :02: ow r56 98 958m am so» SE 8 £33 52: c8: EOE 20% so» 26m vaQ NNQmQ anQ oQOQ mama EQmQ m _ Qmfl seam t 33 n 232 mansim 033839 333$ A330 08 a: mcfima 5 moo—mm wcifim go 823 chwb 33m: :9: waive—m 03:0: 208 BE so» 96m 2:: of mo “woe 952a :3. as.“ B 3.59 H3 98 mcmfixcm E 33285 “.553 6% so» :23» 88: 22: 80>) wage 8: 8:— :£ so» 02% so» H8 Em: :m So x53 8 :58 35: $55 35 gum so» 3mm 38 no» BEE 2:03 32:: 2:: :96 35 0m 6006 can a E 8 638?: 8 38on soon so» 3mm :02 30> >95 2t 50% v.3 :3 so» 262 98.8 E 203 .o be 8 Sons 803 so» 8:— :fl no» 3me 3m bo> coon so» 96m “ma 2: E 3:393 :2: $55 5m 2: a :85?» 3:83 was .95: :2: :85?» Soon coom amo— =£ so» 025 weighs ow r538 no» :2: 20m 20> can? .133 :9: EOE .2853 so :38 soon so» gum N 2QO meD mama mama 5me NQmO :QmQ Emma REG 29mm o—Qmfl seam O: mac n 932 mEOHQE>m 033239 Queues—m _ 28m . om-DmQ oméoime 0Q pom 28m Ema 2; .5 2an 127 3mm. MNb. can. 2%. m 30. m2. mNN. wmo. of. NNo. N 89on «new. «mvm. .omdv was 3.83me 05 88 888 28 :8: 808 :o 388— : 3:83 822% 82? wmv. 825 2: 8 so» 88 voow @850: mm? 82: 98 $280: $3 0.:— 85 so» so» 025 mNQmO 6833. “88— %N. .2883 mam—=8— 8 022% Sons Emacs“ no» 032 ONQmD EN. macaw 8 :88 Sosa 83m: 8:: 208 Ewsofi :9» 3mm VNQmQ o2. got 803 so» its no» 2Q mNQmQ _ as»: a $6 u 282 m8088>m 322% m 2me wEou 803 so» mmfifi 850 so 888 :5» EV. wcaoux .5 £83 8 8.83623 89» 9 82888 mafia :33 82: 05:08 0.58 SE so» 0%: Emma eNv. x83 3 803628 89» on 9 so» 8m Em: v.83 z 85 :38 8 soon so» 083 :DmD 689.2. 2:2— 38:50 038. 128 Subjective Happiness ScalfiSHS; Lyubomirsky & Lepper, 199g). The SHS was used to measure happiness. A CFA of this one-factor measure of happiness was excellent (at: (df2, N=201) = 0.85, p = 0.65, GFI = 0.99, AGFI = 0.99, RMSEA = 0.00, CFI = 1.00, NNFI = 1.01, PNFI = 0.33, PGFI = 0.20]. Reliability of this scale for this current sample was also good (a = 0.88). This scale is represented by SWLS in Figure 11 and high scores reflect greater happiness. Phase Four. Phases 1-3 produced the measurement model (outlines the relationships between the observed variables (measures) and latent variables (theoretical constructs)) which were used in Phase 4 to test the hypothesized relationships between the constructs in the stress-mental health structural model (outlines the hypothesized relationships between the latent variables) and the fit of the data to the model (Hoyle, 1995). Structural Equation Modeling (SEM) analyses of the intercorrelation matrix of the scales produced via EFA (see Table 22) were conducted using LISREL 8.3. Maximum likelihood (ML) estimation was employed to yield optimal parameter estimates. The hypotheses stated above were assessed by examining direct and indirect effects. Each hypothesis was deemed true if the proposed relations were statistically significant and in the predicted direction. The overall fit of the adapted stress-mental health model was determined by assessing, the values of the absolute, relative, and parsimonious fit indices described above. Please refer to the structural and measurement model (Figure 11) for the factor structures, measures, constructs, and pathways in the adapted model. 129 080m 80:153.. 03.00.3sm H 9.5 ”0.00m 0.5 5.3 cosoflmccm H mgm £83983 320:5. H mDmD $59986 028.0500 30.995 u 88 089.8% 068.80 38880 n 58 8580 0529;. u 80m“: 8580 3833 n Comma .880 3.9.8 u 28000 8258 8208 H 8.500 .0580 953. n Emoom 08% 88888.: u 85$ 0380 3530 o. 26 8:222 n 5.32.. m23.5 25:80 .0 .wm8.&a< H Nmmmz Ecuafifitoflfl ..o _am8.&q< H. mmmE £05583. .380ch 0. 9580530.. H mgmmm ”—088.89. 38802—83 002000”. N NmDmmm Juana—3m 3559.3. .380ch $80595 u $.3me ”0.00.8320 800.. H Emma. €9.95 ..00m m82>oum n Emma. ”toga—am .88 82898 n 3:88 888.8 8:3... u 20mm; 8258 has. 8288. u 3:88 55:80 .8 80829.0 n 2282 SE55 .0 cosawooom n 2252 ”mo—mags ::3 8038000m< H N<§mm< Eosfi=0EO 08.04 H 132%"... 30:03:. 05 :o .8250 0.8068000 3:58—3. 0.02 .... 3.. 2.. 2.. 3.. 2.. N... .... .... .... .... ...v 9.. 2.. ...N .... .... .... .... .... .... .... .~.~ :. .... 3.: a. 3.2 2... .... ...: ...: ...: 3... ....~ ...... 2.2 ...: M... 2.: ...: .... ...: ...: ...: .2. ....N 2... ...: .... 3... ...: ...: 8.: m... 3.. ...- ...- a..- 2... 3... .... .... .... 2..- 2..- 2... 2..- 3..- .... .... S... .... .... .... .... 3... .... .... .... ... a .2. 3..- mm... em..- .m... m... .... 2.. 2..- 2... ...... 2..- 2..- mm. .... ...... _... .... .... .... 3... m... 2... .... :3. M.l... v... N... 2.. .... v... :6. 3... 2.. 2.. 3.. .... .... 3..- 3..- m... .... ...... S... 2... a... 2..- 2..- S... 88 a .... M... 3.. ...- 2..- 3... .... 2.. 2.. 3.. M... m... .....- .... ...- 2... Q... .N... 2.. 2.. .....- .... 38 a .... .... .... ...? 3... .... 2.. 2.. .... .... 2... ...- .... 2..- Z... 3.. 3..- .... .... no..- .... 33 ...... M... 2... ...... .... .... .... .... M... N... 2... .... .... 3... 3... .... 2... .... 3... .... 3... 03: a 2... z... 3... .... .... .... .... 2.. 2... 3..- N... 2... z... 2... 3... .... ~... 2... 3... 3.8. a 2.. M... N... 3..- M... .... 3..- .... 2.. 3... .... .... 3..- 2.. 2.. 2.. 3.. :. 39: a 2.. 3..- 2..- 3... 3..- Q..- .... .... .... .... .... .... .... .... 1.. .... 3.. ES. N... .... .... 3.. .... z... .... .... ...... .... .... .... .... 2.. 2.. .... _~.. 3.5.. a .... .... .... z... 2... .... N... 2... z... .... 2... 2.. .... 2... .... $3 a 5.. .... 3..- 2..- 2.. ~... 2...- 3..- .... z... .... .... z..- .... .23 an. S. 3..- Z... .... .... .... 2... ...... 2... .... v... 2... .... a... g .... 2... M... v... .... 2... .... 2... v... .... MN... 2.. ...... a .... .... _m.. 2.... .... N... N... z... 2... 3... 2..- 23.. a .... .....- .... 2.. 2.... ._ .. .... S... .... .... 83... H... .... _ I. 2.. 2... .... .... .... v . ... .... :3... a N... .... .... .... z..- 2..- .....- z... 2:... M... .... 3.. 2.. z... .... 2... .... 2.2 ...]. 2.. 3.. z..- 2.. 2.. 8..- :8... .....I. .... .... .... S..- .... 3?. a .... v... 2.. .... 2.... ml. 3.. 3... a... as: a z..- 3.. 2.: .... z..- 3.3 :4. ::3 .5 :3. 88 88 58 Go... 308. Go... 3.8. 3.8. $3 E3 .3: an: 33$ 83$ 53... 2.... 5:2 :5... N3... 2...... arm: :3: ~55? .552 waged... agoamlflgauo—mxm an 0003005 3.00m 80.. 08065000845233. an? .NN 030... 130 338m 08: Bag. .: 2:3 «Emma :53. 2.. 3&me «Emma o3 . _ .o a... u :58 59: =o=8€> 9.80 820 .8. uh. a: m. n ”x “.082 35858... 3:: u gum. 62. 5.5853 $80 868$ .2». in .32 z n "x U322 .32 mvo o 3... Emma 33mm. 5&5 38m .2 8.. Bioeom Emma ”5.5 R o .Emmm mam Bow"... 36 Nm Comma . . $5. 8... . Nam m... N; end. N0 mg 5.8... 3:22 EBB§8m .o 5.8%? ...:qu 3.0. ”m 2... Memo .3 23 $500.. 2.. 33 $50an _ mEOUm 265850 30330 36365 For hypotheses found to be non-significant, LISREL output was examined to determine if changes needed to be made in the structural and measurement model. Thus, modifications to the measurement model were made by eliminating variables with poor relationships or poor residuals (measurement error), high modification indices for lambda-y (relationships between the latent constructs and the indicators), and high modification indices for theta-epsilon, theta-delta-epsilon, and theta-delta (error covariance between indicators). Modifications to the structural model were made if large modification indices and residuals for beta (relationships between the endogenous variables) and or gamma (relationships between the exogenous variables) indicated that there are parameters that could be estimated (i.e., direct paths) to improve model fit. Results of the Hypotheses Hypothesis 1 A direct, significant positive relationship between individual cultural characteristics and perceived social support was not supported (standardized B = 0.10, t = 1.04, p > 0.20, two-tailed test; Path A, Figure 11). These results indicate that perceived social support is not directly related to individual cultural characteristics. Hypothesis 2 A direct, significant negative relationship was not found between individual cultural characteristics and appraisal of sociocultural stress. However, a direct, positive relationship was found (standardized B = 0.48, t = 5.54, p < 0.001, two-tailed test; Path B, Figure 1 1). These findings suggest that respondents who are more involved in Latina/o culture and who have maintained the language and cultural practices are more likely to experience acculturative stress and/or minority status distress. 132 Hypothesis 3 As predicted, a direct, significant negative relationship was not demonstrated between perceived social support and appraisal of sociocultural stress (standardized B = - 0.13, t = -1.47, p > 0.10, two-tailed test; Path C, Figure 1 1). These results indicate that respondents who perceived less formal and informal support were more likely to experience acculturative stress and/or minority status distress. Hypothesis 4 As predicted a direct, significant positive relationship was demonstrated between individual cultural characteristics and coping (standardized B = 0.26, t = 2.70, p = < 001, two tailed test; Path D, Figure l 1). The results indicate that respondents who have maintained Latina/o cultural practices and the Spanish language are more likely to utilize coping strategies. Hypothesis 5 As predicted, a direct, significant positive relationship between perceived social support and coping was supported (standardized B = 0.47, t = 2.87, p < 0.01, two tailed test; Path E, Figure 11). These results indicate that respondents who perceive the availability of formal and informal social support are more likely to utilize coping strategies. Hypothesis 6 A direct, significant negative relationship was not demonstrated between appraisal of sociocultural stress and coping ((standardized B = -0.05, t = -0.52, p > 0.20, two tailed test; Path F, Figure 11). In other words, the results indicate that respondents who 133 experience less acculturative stress and/or minority status distress are not more likely to utilize coping strategies. Hypothesis 7 A direct, significant positive relationship between coping and mental health was not supported (standardized B = -0.20, t = -2.34 p < 0.02, two tailed test, Path G, Figure 11). These results suggest that respondents who utilize coping strategies were not more likely to experience higher levels of mental health. Hypothesis 8 The indirect relationships hypothesized by Taylor and Aspinwall (1996); individual cultural characteristics to mental health (1] = -0.06, t = -1.93, p > .05) and perceived social support to mental health (1] = -0.09, t = -l.85, p > .05) were not supported. Participants who tended to be bicultural or highly acculturated were not more likely to utilize coping strategies and experience lower levels of mental health. Furthermore, participants who perceived social support were not more likely to utilize coping strategies and report lower levels of mental health. Model 1: Results of the overall adapted Latina/o student stress-mentjal health model This section presents the results of the overall fit of the adapted model and the results of the post hoc analyses on the structural and measurement model that were conducted to improve the overall fit of the model. SEM analyses indicated poor fit for the adapted model [)6 (df 292, N=201) = 851.49, p < 0.001, GFI = 0.75, AGFI = 0.70, RMSEA = 0.10, CFI = 0.70, NNFI = 0.66, PNFI = 0.54, PGFI = 0.63]. Not all ofthe relationships outlined in the proposed model were in the predicted direction (e. g., individual cultural characteristics to appraisal of cultural stress, coping to mental health) 134 and several relationships did not reach statistical significance (e. g., individual cultural characteristics to perceived social support, appraisal of cultural stress to coping). Furthermore, serious measurement problems were evident given the fact that not all factor loadings of each indicator to their respective latent variable were statistically significant (See Table 23 and Figure l 1). As a result, several modifications to the measurement model were made to improve model fit. M09212. As stated above factor loadings of several indicators to their respective latent variable were not statistically significant. Two indicators (i.e., Passive Coping (BCOPE3) and Avoidant Coping (PF SOC2)) did not significantly load on the latent variable, Coping. The literature suggests that there are two broad but distinct coping strategies to reduce psychological distress. There are strategies designed to actively manage, resolve, or influence stressful demands through one’s own efforts (e. g., problem solving, support seeking) and/or strategies designed to adjust to stressful demands by changing the self rather than the situation (e. g., accepting the situation, self-distraction; Cross, 1995). Cross (1995) refers to these distinct coping strategies as Direct Coping and Indirect Coping. In light of the research and non-significant factor loadings of the indicators, BCOPE3 and PFSOC2, the latent variable Coping was divided into two latent variables, Direct Coping and Indirect Coping. The BCOPE3 and PF SOC2 were designated as indicators of Indirect Coping while the remaining three indicators, BCOPE] (Active Coping), BCOPE2 (Seeking Support) and PFSOCl (Assertive Coping) were assigned to the latent variable, Direct Coping. 135 good vd... .885 .E 8 8.28000 $888885 u 50m 585 .E 35.82 808258; u E73 v.85 x... 388.82 n E22 ”:85 5 0388an0 n EU ”829:7.883‘ 8 8:5 898m :32 89m u <35 ”:85 :E 8 88380 3.952 n 30¢. ”82: am ll 8 82880 M ED ..Eo32m 8 823D E amass n 84 8388a 8.83-20 n :3 88305 8 82on n18 w3§cméu n :M 88m 295% u 2 082 NS 8. a. a. 8. 8. N: *8? 8m .28: 8m 98: 888m 85:88:: :2 8. mm. 8. 8. S. :2 .38: SN :58 888m 888:8 82 8. 8. a. no. 8. w 8.: 8m :23 SN :88 85888 3:88 :2 8. 8. S. 2. 8. I *3: :2 .538 SN 8:83 :88 8:88:85 :2 8. a: a. 8. 8. mm: #2:. S: 8583 88.: 8:888 Queer—om :2 8. a. a. 8. 8. N: 8:: 8m .88: a: 98:: 888m 8:88:85 :2 8. 3 3. 3. :w. :2 82$ 8: 28:: 88:8 8:888 on. 8. a. 3. 8. 8. I a. mm mm: :88: 8: 8583 .28.: 8:88:85 :2 8. a. a. 8. 8. :2 :88: SN mms83 :88 888:8 8: m2 8. a. a. 3. 8. N: 8.2 . em: :3: 8: :58 888m 85888:: :2 8. 8. a. 3. 8. :2 8.5, S: :58 888m 8:888 :2 8. 3. 3. 3. E. 2 2 .2 mm: 82:: 8: 8:83 :98”: 85:88:: :2 8. a. 8. 8. 5. mm: 8.8: S: 8:83 :88 888:8 hog—DU 95:20—52 .3 30,—. 8. 8. a. .3. mo. 8. 8. 8: :8: 8: so... 882 8. 8. S. 2. 8. a. S. 8: 1.2.3. 8: 8:8. 882 8. 8. 2. E. 8. 8. a. :2 838 8: :5 882 8. 8. 8. E. S. 2. 3. 8: 3:8: 8: 0:o .88: £2 E75 E22 Eu 522m £9: E0 .6: .8 a M m .982 $282 88825 8:: 88:88:32 :8. 8885 :m 8 828000 .3 033. 136 Three of the indicators of Perceived Social Support, PSSFA2 (Family Intimacy), PSSFR3 (Peer Closeness), and PSSUP (Relationship to University Personnel) did not significantly load on this latent variable. These indicators appear to be a measure of the type of relationship between the respondents and their support network. Given that the literature indicates that perceived support is defined as an individual’s perceptions of general or specific support, available or acted on, from people in their social network (e.g., family, peers, university personnel; Demaray & Malecki, 2002) and is independent of the support network’s characteristics (Eckenrode & Wethington, 1990; Lepore et al., 1991; Sarason et al., 1994), PSSFA2, PSSFR3, and PSSUP3 were removed as indicators of Perceived Social Support. One indicator, ARSMA2 (Association with Anglos) did not load significantly on the latent variable Individual Cultural Characteristics. As stated above, no participants represented a “Very Latina/o Oriented” level, indicating that all participants scored high on the Anglo Orientation Scale of the ARSMA-II. Given these results, only ARSMA] (Latino Orientation), MEIM] (Recognition of Ethnicity), and MEIM2 (Exploration of Ethnicity) remained as indicators of Individual Cultural Characteristics. Although the factors loading for SWLF (Satisfaction with Life) and SHS (Subjective Happiness Scale) on the Mental Health latent variable were significant, these values were negative, unlike the DSD1-3 indicators. As stated above Masse et a1 (1998) have demonstrated that psychological distress and well-being are correlated and are part of a two-dimensional latent construct which reflects a higher-order concept of mental health. Thus, Mental Health was divided into two latent variables, Distress and 137 Wellbeing. SWLF and SHS were assigned as indicators of Wellbeing and DSD1-3 were designated to load on Distress. Table 24 displays the intercorrelation matrix of the remaining scales used to test Model 2. These modifications to the measurement model yielded an improvement in model fit (see Table 23); however, the overall model fit remained poor [)6 (df 198, N=201) = 508.32, p < 0.001, GFI = 0.81, AGFI = 0.76, RMSEA = 0.09, CFI = 0.79, NNFI = 0.76, PNFI = 0.61, PGFI = 0.62]. The output suggested that several structural changes could be made to improve model fit. Please see Figure 12. Respecification of this model was guided by both theoretical and empirical considerations. Below are descriptions of the respecification steps that were taken to improve model fit. Mgdgll Model 2 was respecified to eliminate the following non significant paths. Since the literature indicates that bicultural and highly acculturated Latina/o university students use mostly direct coping approaches (Mena et al., 1987; Vazquez & Garcia-Vazquez, 1995), the path from Individual Cultural Characteristics to Indirect Coping was eliminated. The path from Appraisal of Sociocultural Stress to Direct Coping was found to be non significant and was eliminated. The literature also indicates that individuals may cope with cultural stress both indirectly (Crocker & Major, 1989; Phenice & Griffore, 1994; Schmader et al., 2001) and directly (Phinney & Chavira, 1995; Mena et al., 1987). However, the appraisal of sociocultural stress appears to influence an individual’s type of coping strategies (Schmader et al., 2001). This sample of respondents reported perceiving acculturative stress and/or minority status stress as slightly stressful. 138 030m mm0fimaam 03:00:95 .I. wIm ”0:05 0.2.: 5:3 comuommflamm n mas/w MESQEm 33026 n mDmO £89993 023033: $03.25 N NDmQ $883.3 0230309 3.5325: n :QmQ MmEQoU 5qu3: u NUOmmm ”wagon: 0235: u mmmOUm m::aoo SE82 u Comm: £953 :58: M 8:80 05:00 3:3. u Emoom 08:: .283885 u ~03: 03:30 3530 2 26 8%55. ... 5.2m M225 8950? 38%? u 382 aofiafiaafi .8 Emma? n. 3:: 205580“: 368203 003003: u NmDmmm ”togsm 39:880.": 38:02.83 250305 N EDmmm €895 :00: $58.65 n ”Emma: e935 5»: 328...: u ::3: £955 58: 8289: u 2:8: 5665”: :0 gaseoaxm n 2252 .fimoafim mo :oEcwooom n 32:92 6058520 0:20.: .I. :Szg sacommfi 05 :0 .8250 080208000 @6933: .80 Z :3 8.: _~._ 8.: 8.: 8.: 8.: 8.: 8.: :3 :3 :3 8.: 2.: 8.: 8.: 2.: 8.: 8.: :3 ...: 8.: 3 ::.:~ 2.8 3.: 8.2 8.:. 8.8 8.2 8.2 8.:. 8.8 8.2 8.: 8.: 8.2 :3. 8.2 2.1 8.:: 8.3 8.: 3.1 8.8 :8: :.:.|: 8.: ::.? 3.? 8.:. 2.:. 8.:- 8.: 1.: 2.: 8.:- 2.:. 2.:. :~.? 2.: 2.: 2.: 2.: 8.: 8.:. 8.: 1.: 2.: :l:.: 8.:. 8.:. 8.? 2.? 3.? 2.: _~.: 2.: 2.:- :~.? 2.:. 2.:. 2.: 1.: 2.: 8.: 3.: 8.? 8.: 8.: :3: a 8.: 8.: 2.: ::.: 8.: ...? 8.:- :~.: 1.: 1.: 2.: 8.:- 8.:. 8.:. 2.:. 8.:. 8.: 2.:. 8.:. 88 84:. 8.: 8.: 8.: 8.? 8.:. 8.:- 8.: 2.: 8.: 2.: 8.:. 8.:. 8.? 2.? 8.:. 2.: 2.: 8.: ::3 a: 3.: 8.: 8.? 8.:- 8.:- :m.: 2.: 8.: 2.: :~.? 8.:. 2.? 2.? 2.? 8.: 8.: 8.: .3: 8.: 8.: 8.:. 8.: 8.: 8.: :~.: 2.: 2.: 2.? 8.: 8.:. 8.:. 2.:. 8.: 8.:. 8.:. 8:2: ::3 2.:. ...? 8.? 8.: 8.: :~.: 2.: 2.:- 8.:. 2.:. 2.? 8.:. 8.: 8.: 8.:. 8:8: 3.: 8.: 8.: 8.: 8.? 8.: 8.: :m: 8.: 8.: 8.: 8.: 8.: 2.: 2.: 5:2: 8.: 8.: 8.:. 2.? 8.:. 8.? 2.: 8.: 8.: 8.: 3.: 8.: 1.: 8.: 3:8: 8: 2.: 8.: 8.: 8.: :~.: 1.: 8.: 2.: 2.: 8.: 8.: 8.: ::8: H1: 8.: 8.: 8.: 2.:. 8.: 8.:. 1.:. 2.:. 8.: :m.: 2.: $3 84: 8.: 8.: 2.:. 2.: 8.:. 8.? 8.:. _ .8 8.: 2.: :3 a 8.: _ .8. 8.: 8.: 8.? 2.:. 8.: 8.: 8.: 82: 3.: 2.? 8.: 8.: 8.:. 2.? 8.: 8.: 8.: :2: a“: 8.: 8.: 8.: 2.: 8.: 8.? 8.: ~82: :|:..: _ .8 2.: 8.: 8.: 8.: 8.: .82: .84: 8.: 8.: 8.: 8.: 2.: 2:2: 8.: 8.: 8.? 8.: 8.:. 8:2: :4: 2.: 1.: 2.: .12: a 8.: 2...: 2.:: a: 8.: En: a: 2:92 2.: 23: 88 38 38 302: 3:8: 28:: ~23: :5: 2:22 a? 8m: 0.28: 5: $86880 333% .8 03a: 139 933: N 382 858%: .2 2:3: a: u :58 565 8:883 320 389.0 :2 in .28: u ”x ”.082 80358:: E 8.2 130m: 52. 8.58.1.3 320 88820 .1 2 n8 .882 n Nx :08: ==z _ 8mm _ _ Smfl Q81— 3. mmEOUm NOOmn—m oflmtofigu 2.: a; 3.: S .o. ”m .8330 to who :9: 3:33.05 :gwg 9:000 805.: 9.. :Ew 805m 3.: 8530203 mmmmE Egg? .... I mde 5me was? +l| .55 Ba I :8 8.: 2.: wad ”m tannw 785?: a: 8.: .008: So 38m 2:: «he 3302K "85$ wed _ mmOum NmmOUm _ 35me Ed 3.: Ngmmm _ 95:: __ :28: _ 140 Because the respondents perceived relatively low levels of sociocultural stress, it is hypothesized that they were less likely to take action to reduce stress (i.e., discussing it with others, disproving stereotypes, using self affirmation). Therefore, they were not likely to utilize direct coping strategies. The relationship between Individual Cultural Characteristics to Perceived Social Support was eliminated because it was found to be non significant. The few studies that have examined the relationship between individual cultural characteristics and perceived social support (Griffith & Villavicencio, 1985; Sabogal et al., 1987) have found that there is a relationship between these two variables. However, these researches have failed to distinguish between network characteristics (i.e., large support network) and perceived social support. Furthermore, some (e. g., Sabogal et al., 1987) have found that perceived family support remains high despite acculturation level. The relationship between Appraisal of Sociocultural Stress and Perceived Social Support was non significant. Although the literature does not support it, this path was eliminated. Model 2 was also respecified to include a direct path fi'om Distress to Wellbeing because this path had a large modification index for beta and some of the residuals between the indicators of these two variables were high. This modification is consistent with previous literature. Eliminating the non significant paths fi‘om Model 2 and adding a direct path from Distress to Wellbeing produced an improvement in fit [x2 (df 200, N=201) = 439.27, p < 0.001, GFI = 0.83, AGFI = 0.79, RMSEA = 0.08, CFI = 0.85, NNFI = 0.82, PNFI = 0.66, PGFI = 0.66]. 141 me. One of the Appraisal of Sociocultural Stress indicators, SAP E2, was eliminated from Model 3. SAFE2 presented several problems for the model including high residuals and high modification indices for lambda-y (i.e., model fit would improve if SAFE2 was allowed to load on other latent variables; Indirect Coping and Distress). Although SAFE2 was moderately correlated with several of the Indirect Coping and Distress indictors (i.e., the correlations were near 0.30), it was not theoretically justifiable to allow SAFE2 to be an indicator of these two constructs. Thus, SAFE2 was removed as an indicator of Individual Cultural Characteristics. A review of the modification indices for theta-epsilon, theta-delta-epsilon, and theta-delta revealed that the error covariance between several of the indicators could be estimated to improve model fit. The error covariances between PSSF R1 and PSSFR2, PSSUP] and PSSUP2, DSDl and DSD3, and BCOPE3 and SHS were estimated. In addition, the error covariances between PSSFRI and each of the indicators of Direct Coping (e. g., BCOPE], BCOPE2, PF SOCl) were also estimated. According to several researchers (e. g., Byrne, 1998, Joreskog & Sorbom, 1993), the specification of correlated error terms for purposes of achieving a better fitting model must be supported by a strong substantive rational, empirical rationale, or both. Therefore, the covariance between PSSFR] and PSSFR2 and PSSUP] and PSSUP2 is justified as substantively meaningfiil given that perceived social support is a multidimensional construct with correlated dimensions that include social networks, received social support, and provided social support (Cohen et al., 1986; Eckenrode & Wethington, 1990; Hobfoll & Vaux, 1993; Lepore et al., 1991; Sarason et al., 1994 Vaux, 142 1988). Similarly, the estimation of the covariance between DSD] and DSD3 is conceivable given that symptoms of depression and suicide have been found to be highly correlated in the literature (e. g., D’Zurilla et al., 1998; Essau &Trommsdorff, 1996; Leong et al., 1997; Nezu et al., 1986). Furthermore, the covariance of BCOPE3 and SHS is supported given the fact that passive coping has been found to be related to lower levels of happiness and higher levels of depression and hopelessness (Clum, & Febbraro, 1994; D’ Zurilla et al., 1998; Essau & Trommsdorff, 1996; Leong et al., 1997; Nezu & Ronan, 1988; Priester & Clum, 1993). Finally, the estimation of error covariance between PSSFRl and each of the indicators of Direct Coping (e. g, BCOPE], BCOPE2, PFSOCl) is justified given that perceived social support has been found to precede, influence, and assist effective coping efforts (Billings & Moos, 1981; Cohen, 1992; Cronkite & Moos, 1984; Holahan & Moos, 1986, 1987; Holahan et al., 1997; Lazarus & Folkman, 1984; Lepore et al., 1991; Thoits, 1986; 1995). Table 25 contains the intercorrelation matrix of the remaining scales used to test Model 4. These modifications produced a better fitting model [)8 (df 167, N=201) = 241.67, p > 0.001, GFI = 0.90, AGFI = 0.86, RMSEA = 0.05, CFI = 0.94, NNFI = 0.93, PNFI = 0.68, PGFI = 0.65]. Furthermore, all direct pathways outlined in this model were statistically significant (p <0.05) and the factor loading of each indicator to its respective latent variable in this model were all statistically significant (p < 0.05). Please see Figure 13. Model 4 differs from the adapted model, Model 1, in that this model suggests that indirect coping and direct coping are two separate coping constructs. Similarly, Model 4 suggests that distress and wellbeing are two separate mental health constructs. Model 1 I43 28m 383:3: 0280.35 n mmm 6.8m £3 5.3 8.800an u 53m m......erim .863 n 88 089993 02000509 30:35": N NDmQ ”macaakm 0230.500 :gocOEm: H :59 $5000 “000.03% M NUOmmm 0530 308: u 8:000 0580 9.08.2 n 509:: £253 meioom u 8:000 0500 9:3. n .3000 meow—00m: .8330 9 0:0 00:30.2 n :Em ”0.52:0 0.09000 .«0 18.5.3.0. u Nmmmz onacmatommfl :0 EMEAEAV u—mmmE €5.83: 30.955 .3283: u gamma ”noun—am 3583: 00.83:: 9:38.: u 30mm: £285 .8: 052.65 u Emma 5&8 so: 8383: u 20mm: €2.05 :28... 09.83: u 20mm: 3.055 :0 soaeoaxm u «Sam—2 bmoE—tmuo aowlawoogm u ENE “00:00:00.00 0525 n «(.Emfi ”@085: 05 :0 309:0 300.0580 Emma—50¢ .80? ::.: 3.: _~.. ::.: .:.: ::.: 3.: ::.: 2.: ::.: ::.: 3.: 2.: _:.v ::.: :..m :4: .v: :~.~ ...m 3... :m ::.:N 2.: 3.: ::.:. ::.:. ::.: ::.:. ::.- ::.:. ::.MN ::.: ::.: ::.:. ::.:. ::.~. :~.:. 3.3 .:.:v ::.: 3.2 as: :8: a 3.: ::.:- ::.:- ::.:. ::.:. 3.? ::.: 2.: 2.: m..:. 2.? :~.:. 2.: ...: 2.: 2.: ::.: .:.:. ::.: 1.: 2.: a NM? 3.? ::.:. ::.:- ::.:- 2.: .~.: 2.: :~.:- 2.? 2.:. ::.: 2.: .m.: 2.: ::.: ::.:. ::.: v:.: ::3: M:|.: 3.: 3.: :..: ::.: 3.: ...? ::.:. 3.: 2.: ...: ::.:. ::.:. ::.:. 2.:- 2.:- ::.: 2.? S? ::3 fl ::.: :1: 3.: ::.:. ::.:. v:.:. 2.: 3.: 2.: ::.:. ::.:. ::.:. 3.? 2.:. 2.: 3.: ::.: 3:: a ::.: ::.: ::.:- 3.? v:.:. 2.: ::.: 2.: ::.:- ::.:. ~..? ...? 2.:. ::.: ::.: ::.: .3: ~|:d ::.: ::.:. ::.: ::.: :~.: :..: 2.: 2.:. ::.: 8.? v:.:. 2.:- v:.: ::.:. v:.:. 03:: Si: 2.:. ...? 3.? 2.: ::.: :..: «_.:. ::.:. 2.? ...? 3.? ::.: ~:.: 3.? 3:8: g 2.: ::.: ::.:. ::.: ::.: ::.: 2.: 2.: ::.: -.: .~.: :..: ...: 63:: mgr. ::.: 2.:. v:.:. ::.:- 2.: ::.: ::.: 3.: ::.: ::.: 2.: 3.: 3:8: 31: ::.: ::.: ::.: :~.: 2.: ::.: :..: 2.: 2.: 2.: .~.: .::8: Nada. ::.: ::.: 2.:- S: v:.:. v:.:. 3.? ...: ::.: 2.: ::3 an: ::.: ...? ::.: .:.: ::.:. :..:. ::.: ::.: ::.: $3: a 2.:. ::.: ::.: ::.:. ~..:. 3.: ~m.: 2.: 33: ....dd ::.: 2.: 2.: :..: ::.: ::.:. ::.: ::3: a . _.: 2.: v:.: ::.: ::.: ::.: ::3: 5.1.: 3.: ::.: ::.: ::.: 2.: 2:3: a .m.: ~:.? ::.: ::.:. ::3: .I:l.:. 2.: 3.: 2.: 2:3: Si: 2.: 2.: 2.:... m|:ud 3.: .23: fl 2:92 2.: ::3: m3: 3:: .3: 03:: 3:8: 5:3: 3:8: 3:8: 3:3 ::3: .3: 23:: ::3: 2:3: ...:3: .<:$:~i:.:z £555.92 v .0002 a. 00m: 3.00m 00.: 300.0500qb..50=0m 03003500000030: .mm 030:. I44 a: n 96m: 505 8§=§ 020 389.0 .5: ".0 .32.“ u "x “082 §§e§: 830$ 320 0280.”: a: "a. 03:: u ax ”.822 :24 038: v 382 .2 2am dam: «Na 1508 59: a... mud gum—E 830830 @330 36305 mmmoum 80::: n2 . . 28 cod K c 8 o macaw 826:: $055 ::.: u: 8.9 2.? H: S .o HO ::.:. ”I 33:03 0:80 025 NS . ::.: um S c «we 58:: 26 :m 0 as 9.5 ...—gm NNAOUm n: Um ::.: .o f 223 3o :92: mmmmz cg Exazm :53: 38: 0038.0: 23 $5 A 8.3mm: «2. 2mm: 2:3: 145 proposed that Individual Cultural Characteristics directly influenced Perceived Social Support and that Perceived Social Support directly influenced Appraisal of Sociocultural Stress. Model 4 indicates that these relationships are not significant, thus these paths were removed. Model 4 also indicates a direct significant positive relationship between Individual Cultural Characteristics and Appraisal of Sociocultural Stress. However this path is not in the predicted direction (standardized B = 0.44, t = 5.08, p < 0.001, two- tailed test; Path E, Figure 13). Paths D (standardized B = 0.22, t = 2.71, p <0.01, two- tailed test; Figure 13), E (standardized B = 0.48, t: 3.89, p <0.001, two-tailed test; Figure 13), F (standardized B = 0.32, t = 3.70, p <0.001, two-tailed test; Figure 13), and G (standardized B = 0.17, t = 2.48, p <0.02, two-tailed test; Figure 13) of Model 1 were found to be statistically significant in the hypothesized direction in Model 4. Model 4 also contains several paths that were not hypothesized for Model 1. These include Path H (standardized B = -0.35, t = 3.01, p < 0.01, two-tailed test; Figure 13), Path I (standardized B = 0.81, t = 8.12, p <0.001, two-tailed test; Figure 13), and Path J (standardized B = -0.73, t: -9.05, p <0.001, two-tailed test; Figure 13). Furthermore, consistent with the literature, several indicators were removed from the hypothesized model and the covariance of several pairs of indicators was estimated. Seven statistically significant indirect relationships were demonstrated. Indirect relationships from Individual Cultural Characteristics to Indirect Coping (n = 0.14, t = 3.09, p < .001) and Individual Cultural Characteristics to Distress (n = 0.12, t = 3.09, p < .01) were found. Participants who tended to be bicultural or highly acculturated were more likely to experience sociocultural stress, utilize indirect coping strategies and experience distress. Indirect relationships fi'om Perceived Social Support to Distress (n = 146 -0.28, t = -3.00, p < .01) and to Wellbeing (n = 0.29, t = 3.45, p <.001) were found. Perceived social support promoted direct coping strategies which in turn promoted wellbeing. In addition, perceived social support also promoted indirect coping strategies which promoted distress and ultimately affected wellbeing. Indirect relationships between Appraisal of Sociocultural Stress to Distress (n = 0.26, t = 3. 69, p <.001) and to Wellbeing (n = -0. 19, t = -3.50, p < .001) and Indirect Coping to Wellbeing (n = -0.59, t = -6.74, p <.001) were also found. Participants who experienced sociocultural stress utilized indirect coping strategies, experienced distress, and reported lower levels of wellbeing. Moderators The variables, gender, SES, and heritage were tested as moderators. First, a test of measurement invariance was performed. Thus, analyses were conducted to see if the indicators assessed the same latent variables in different groups. The evaluation of measurement invariance was done by comparing the relative fit with the xzdmame test of two models, one with cross-group equality constraints imposed on the factor loading and the other without constraints. Ifthe fit of a model with equality-constrained loadings was not significantly worse than that of the unconstrained model, then it was assumed that the indicators measured the factors in comparable ways in each group (Byrne, 1998; Kline, 1998). After the equality of factor structures was assessed, analyses were conducted to determine if group membership (i.e., male vs. female, low SES vs. high SES, mono- ethnic heritage (two parents of Latino heritage) vs. bi-ethnic heritage (one Non-Latina/o parent and one parent of Latina/o heritage) moderated the structural paths (relationships) specified in the model. Thus, cross-group equality constraints were imposed on both the 147 factor loadings and the structural paths (Byrne, 1998). The x2 of the model with its factor loadings and structural paths constrained to equality was then contrasted against that of the unconstrained model. If the xzdmmnce test of the two models was significant, it was concluded that the structural paths differed across the groups (Byrne, 1998; Kline, 1998). QQLdQI Tables 26 and 27 display the intercorrelation matrices, means, and standard deviations used to test differences between males and females, respectively. The evaluation of measurement invariance revealed that the factor loadings were the same for males (N=72) and females (N_=129). The fit of the model with equality-constrained loadings [12 (df352, N=201) = 402.92, p < 0.001, GFI = 0.87, RMSEA = 0.04, CFI = 0.95, NNFI = 0.94, PNFI = 0.66, PGFI = 1.32] was not significantly worse (xzmqmnce test p = 0.16) than that ofthe unconstrained model [x2 (df 338, N=201) = 383.79, p < 0.001, GFI = 0.87, RMSEA = 0.04, CFI = 0.95, NNFI = 0.94, PNFI = 0.64, PGFI = 1.27]. Please see Table 23. We can assume that the indicators measure the factors in comparable ways in each group. Given that the factor loadings were invariant across males and females, as suggested by Byrne (1998) a test of group differences in the means of the latent constructs was conducted. This test revealed that females exhibited statistically significant higher levels than males on Perceived Social Support (K = 2.32, t = 2.69, p < 0.01, D = -0.57), Distress (K = 1.89, t = 2.84, p<0.01, D = -0.55), and Wellbeing (K = 2.03, t = 4.04, p<0.001, D = -0.96). Statistically significant differences were not evident for Individual Cultural Characteristics (K = 1.95, t = 1.43, p > 0.10, D = -0.23), Appraisal of Sociocultural Stress (K = 0.52, t = -0.46, p > 0.20, D = -0.08), Direct Coping (K = -0.52, t = -0.77, Q > 0.20, D = 0.14), or Indirect Coping (K = 1.04, t = 1.96, p 148 28m 8835mm 9.80.53 n 9% ...-.38 £3 5? 808383 n 55% wage 32on u mama ..mEoEEam 38290 .835 n mama gage 3.86800 EconoEm u 5mm ..wfiaoo 5863.. n 80.8.5 8580 2,88 u ammoom .3800 3053‘ n COPE €855 msxoom n «Boom 3:80 953. u 5808 $8.8 5868885 u 83 38830 9 0% eon-.53. "ES .2330 395.0% .8853. u «mam—2 .. 80585 .8 3853. u 532 ..EEBBA savages 888m n Ebmwm €895 €588.” barge: 9.60.65 u Ebmmm €955 boa managed u Emma ..coaazm boa 8>B8m n “Emma 8896 sigh egg u 33.5mm ..bsafim 0o zonauoaxm u 352 mbzogm ..o Engaged n _Emuz 38855 05.3 u 22% ..Ecomee 05 so 586% 88888... hafnium .302 Nmé 00.0 8.0 006 :6 0:. 00." 0N.0 0N.m 0N.0 0; v0.0 0N.0. NNé de 0m.m 2.0 .06 0N.N N5” 8.0. a 00.0. 2.2 3.." 00.2 8.: 00.2 N02 v0.NN 3:0 MN.NN 5.0 3.2 8.2 00.: NY". ...—.0. 0a.: NNNM 00.0 0N.0. 3.3 :3: 00.0 3.0 0N.0- 3.0- 0W0- 0N.0. 5.0- 0N.0 2.0 3.0 3.0. N0.0- 0.0. 3.0 __.0 2.0 _0.0- Nm.0 N00. .00 2.0 «:0 00.0 mN.0- 3.0- $10- 3.0- 3.0.. 0N.0 N00 __.0 2.0- __.0- 2.0. 2.0 0.0 ...—.0 v0.0 .00 00.0- 00.0 2.0 “:30 0N.0 Nm.0 3.0 0N.0 2.0 8.0- 2.0- 8.0 2.0 0N.0 N..0 8.0. 8.0. 2.0 2.0. Nm.0- 2.0 0.0. 8.0 88 v0.0 :0 0m.0 8.0 N_.0- 8.0- 8.0- 2.0 MN.0 0N.0 NN.0- 3.0. 00.0- 00.0. mN.0- 2.0 _0.0 8.0 N08 8.0 8.0 3.0 2.0. 8.0- 8.0 «Nd NN.0 2.0 :0. 00.0- .0.0. _0.0- :0. 0N.0 0.0 2.0 .000 00.0 3.0 8.0- _0.0- 8.0 Nm.0 0N.0 2.0 vN.0- 2.0 00.0 __.0- 8.0- 00.0 2.0- 8.0. 002.. 2.0 3.0- 2.0- __.0 0N.0 0N.0 2.0 2.0. 8.0 _0.0- 2.0. 2.0. __.0 8.0 8.0 80000 00.0 3.0 00.0 8.0 00.0 8.0- 0N.0 .N.0 2.0 N00 :0 2.0 2.0 3.0 .03”: 00.0 N10 00.0 3.0- _ 0.0 00.0 00.0 0N.0 N10 000 m _ .0 m _ .0 0 _ .0 80000 8.0 N _ .0 2 .0 00.0 8.0 N _ .0 NN.0 00.0 0 _ .0 3.0 _ N.0 0. .0 30000 00.0 00.0 0N.0 0_ .0- 0N.0 v _ .0 m _ .0 00.0- 0 _ .0 8.0 m _ .0 .03 00.0 2.0 E .0- 00.0 N _ .0 8.0- N _ .0- 3.0 and 0N.0 Nam: 8.0 0N.0. v0.0 8.0 8.0 00.0- N _ .0 0N.0 NN.0 _ an: 00.0 00.0 MN.0 . N.0 3.0 8.0- N . .0- 80 No.38 00.0 m _ .0 N . .0 8.0- I .0- 00.0- 00.0- Ear. 8.0 00.0 m _ .0 00.0 8.0. _ _ .0 N508 00.0 0 _ .0 00.0- 8.0- 8.0. .502 3.0 n _ .0 8.0 8.0 .508 00.0 8.0 00.0 N50: 8.0 NN.0 .23: 00.0 _<:2< «:0 “:30 88 N08 .08 N000“: 80000 .000”... N380 $0000 .03 Nam: .03: N002 2030 N552 3:2 .12 N50: ...-0: ::g 332 com $568000 EELS—um use mGOUEDhOOBEH .oN Bonu- 149 23m 88.85 380.3% n mmm 6.8m 0.5 5.3 88.808 n "33m ..manEAm 3.63m u 88 ”acoaesm 3 8280 .8338 u 88 gnaw 3.8289 8268mm n 58 @080 .5295. n «comma ..mcaoo 838.. u 8.50m 8.80 0358... n 509.3 ..coeaam 95.8w u ammoom ..ano 9:3. u 5.52. .885 888825 u 8.2m ...2330 9 26 8:832 u 551% .8330 30980.00 13.895. u wamz .. 8.03.50.85.00 13.5594. H _wmmz 2055me 5.80350 voiooom H gmmm 0.55.9.5 .occofiom 383:5 «536$ n EDmmm €885 8m 8632.. u Sim?“ 8255 son 888m u .Emmm ..toaaam 3.83 838... n summed 5.25:”; .6 consoaxm u «SEE SEEM“? 52583. u _Smmz 62.3500 8:3 u 229?. 29895. as 8 38% «086880 bzséom .082 00.0 8.0 00.. 00.0 0N.0N MNéN 8.0 3.2 a 00.0 NN.0. 00.0- § NN.0. 00.0- a 8.0 a a... “:30 88 N08 .08 N00”... 8800 .03“: N880 .8000 .33 N38 .3": N..0?— ..=.ur. NE?— ....3.. .508 N50... ape: m 98 8o: z .c as E .2 28h _ ..0 00.0N 00.0- N0.0- 8.0 00.0 8.... 8.0 00....N .N.0- NN.0- N ..0 0N.0 N00 N-N-d 0:. .3. 00.0- 3.0- 0N.0 00.0 NWO ......0 a 8.0 0 _.NN 8.0- 00.0 8.0 00.0- N0.0- N0.0- . ..0- mm. 0..n 0N. . . N..0 0N.0 . ..0- 0 ..0- N ..0- 3.0 N ..0- 3.0 34. 00.0 0N.NN 0N.0 0N.0 00.0- 3.0- 0 . .0. N0.0- 00.0- 00.0 0m. Nd... 0N.M de 0..0- 0N.0- m ..0 m ..0 0N.0 NN.0 NN.0 N ..0- .N.0- 00.0. “.0... N00 .0.N. 0N.0- .N.0- . ..0 0N.0 0N.0 m . .0 m . .0 8.0 N0.0- 8.0 8.0 0%. 0m. 0 00.0. 0N.0- 2.0. . ..0 0N.0 .N.0 N ..0 0 ..0 «..0 00.0. 8.0 Nv.0 00.0 3.1.. 2.0 8.0. 0N.0 8.0 00.0. 8.0- 0N.0. 0 ..0. v ..0. 0N.0 NN.0 0N.0 0N.0- 00.0- 0 ..0. 3.1.. .N.m 0N.N. N . .0 0 . .0 .0.0- 00.0- N0.0- 00.0 N ..0. NN.0 00.0 N . .0 N0.0- 00.0 .0.0 8.0 Mad 0N.N No.0 . Nn.0 00.0 0N.0. m ..0- 00.0. 00.0- NN.0- 0N.0 8.0 0N.0 v ..0. 00.0- N0.0- NN.0 0 . .0 a 006 8.NN 0M0 3.0 0N.0- 0N.0- 0N.0. 3.0- m . .0. N ..0 00.0 NN.0 2.0. 0 . .0. 8.0- 0.... 0 .N.0 00.0 a N0.N 0...; 8.0 8.0 0N.0. NN.0- 0N.0. 0N.0- 3:0. N ..0 .m. 0 0 ..0 8.0- N..0- N..0- 0N.0 m ..0 00.0 NN.0 ..0... .N.N 8.0 00.0 .0.0- 00.0 N . .0 00.0 8.0 N0.0- NN.0 00.0 mN.0 . ..0 0N.0 NN.0 m . .0 N . .0 0. .0 3.0 m..0 a 0N... 0N.0. 00.0 00.0 ... ..0- 00.0 8.0. 00.0. 00.0- 00.0 00.0 0N.0 o _ .0 Rd 8.0 8.0 m _ .0 N . .0 00.0 0 . .0 00.0 ...»..d 8.. . 0 ..0m 0 ..0 N0.0- 8.0- 00.0 .0.0 8.0- 00.0- v ..0 00.0- 0N.0 v ..0 00.0 0N.0 00.0 N ..0 3.0 N0.0- 0 ..0 00.0 N00 8 .<=2< 0n :8: 80 “...?” 88 N08 .08 N000... 3.800 .00.... N880 .8000 .03 Na”: .03: N808 Ear. N58. .53.. .538 N50: .50: .532 150 = 0.05, D = -0.45). Tables 26 and 27 indicate that these differences are also confirmed in observed variables. The relative fit of the model with its factor loadings and structural paths constrained to equality [)8 (df 358, N=201) = 427.48, p > 0.001, GFI = 0.86, RMSEA = 0.04, CFI = 0.94, NNFI = 0.93, PNFI = 0.66, PGFI = 1.33] was not worse (xzdmemce test was significant (p = 0.93)) than that of the unconstrained model [)8 (df 336, N=201) = 414.41, p > 0.001, GFI = 0.86, RMSEA = 0.05, CFI = 0.93, NNF1= 0.92, PNFI = 0.63, PGFI = 1.25]. Thus, we can conclude that the structural paths do not differ for males and females and gender is not a moderator of the respecified model. Please see Table 23. Socioeconomic Status SES levels (high and low) were determined by combining information from two items of the demographic questionnaire (i.e., comparison of participants’ families’ financial status with other MSU students’ families; parents’ education level). Participants were assigned to the high SES group if they reported that they were about the same, better off or much better off than other MSU students’ families and their mother’s or father’s highest education level was greater than high school. Participants were assigned to the low SES group if they reported that they were about the same, somewhat worse off or much worse off than other MSU students’ families and their mother’s and father’s highest education level was at most high school. Participants who reported that they were somewhat worse off or much worse off and their father or mother had above a high school education, were also assigned to the low SES group. Similarly, participants who reported that they were better off or much better off and their father or mother had below a high school education were assigned to the high SES group. 151 Tables 28 and 29 display the intercorrelation matrices, means, and standard deviations used to test differences between low and high SES groups, respectively. The evaluation of measurement invariance revealed that the factor loadings were the same for the high (l_\{=7l) and low (fl=130) SES groups. The fit of the model with equality- constrained loadings [x2 (df352, N=201) = 390.58, p < 0.001, GFI = 0.89, RMSEA = 0.03, CFI = 0.94, NNFI = 0.93, PNFI = 0.65, PGFI = 1.35] was not significantly worse (78.1mm... test p = 0.05) than that of the unconstrained model [)52 (df 338, N=201) = 366.66, p < 0.001, GFI = 0.89, RMSEA = 0.03, CFI = 0.95, NNFI = 0.94, PNFI = 0.64, PGFI = 1.30]. We can assume that the indicators measure the factors in comparable ways in each group. Please see Table 23. A test of group differences in the means of the latent constructs revealed that compared to the low SES group, high SES participants exhibited statistically significant higher levels on Perceived Social Support (K = 0.97, t = 2.23, p <0.05, D = -0.83) and Wellbeing (K = 1.67, t = 2.52, g< 0.02, D = -0.51) and lower levels on Appraisal of Sociocultural Stress (K = -1 .38, t = -2.98, p < 0.01, D = 0.48). Statistically significant differences were not evident on Individual Cultural Characteristics (K = -0.19, t = -0.35, p > 0.20, D = -0.06), Direct Coping (K = -0.66, t = - 0.88 p > 0.20, D = 0.18), Indirect Coping (K = 1.50, t = 1.54, p > 0.10, D = -O.32), and Distress (K = -0.04, t = -O.48, p > 0.20, D = 0.11). Tables 28 and 29 indicate that these differences are confirmed in observed variables. 152 280 $8.080 258.35 n mam 808m 03 5.3 8:20QO n .526 058.05% 3830 n 33 aeoasam 03300000 100.030 H N0m0 ”0:030:03 030000000 300.0050 H .0m0 $0.000 0:00.0>< H NUOmnE $0.000 0>.mm0m N 000000 m0580 3.082 u .000”: £895 museum n $0000 m00.80 959.. n. 50000 885 38888:. u 80% 600.000 .8330 00 0:0 00.0000? H .0043 ”003.00 000500 .00 .3053». H NwmmS. 80.000.00.300 .00 02.00000. ".mmmz 000000000 5.000983 0020000 u Nmemm mtoamnw 00000000 3.9.0300 80.03000 H .0058. ...—0095 0000 $0.030...— ” gmmmm 0803 .80 82080 n gamma €895 .0050 82800 n 3052 5.0.5008 80829.0 u 9:02 3.0.5.00 .00 00.0.:w000m H .253. ”00.000.00.00 00.00.. H _émfi 40008.0 05 :0 000000 300.0508 5:30:00 .000 Z 00.0 00.0 00.. 00.0 N00 00.0. 0..NN 00.0 00.2 00.2 31.10 N00 80. N..0. 000. 0.040 0N0. N00. 80. ad .N.0 00.0 30 N00 No.10 m..N 0N.0N 30. 000- 0N0 000 .00 a 0N.0 N0.0— Nv0. NN.0- 000 30 N00 00.0 81.. 8.0 00.0N 00.0 00.0 0.0 80. N00 .00. 0 _ .0. mm... 0N.0 N00. 80. 000 0.0 N..0 0N.0 N..0 N00. 0N.0 .lqlo 00.0 0N.NN 00.0 80 V0.0 0.0 000 N00 80 30 0N0 3 No.0 0..N N00. 0N.0. . ..0 N00 0N0 vN0 0N.0 N..0 N00- «.0 000 N00 00.0. 0 . .0. 0 ..0. 0N.0 NN.0 0N.0 N. . .0 .N.0 000 8.0. 000 000 _Imd 0.0. 00.: N ..0. N _0. _ ..0 0.0 0N.0 0.0 0.0 00.0 8.0. 0.0 $0 000 a 0 _ .... N00. 0 _ .0 00.0 8.0. 00.0. 0N.0. 0N.0. 0 ..0- Z0 8.0 000 80- N..0. . ..0- ~01... 00.0 N . .N . N00. 80 _ ..0 0 . .0. N00. 00.0 00.0 0N0 N00 000 0N.0 80 v . .0 N00 wad 000 00.0. v . .0 00.0 80 0N.0 N ..0 0 . .0 v0.0- 0N.0 0 . .0 NN.0 80 8.0 .00 N . .0 0.0 a 8.0 3.: N00. 000. 00.0. 0N.0 v . .0 m ..0 .00. 000. 3.0 00.0 000 00.0 000 N00 .00. 000 Nd... N00 MN.N0 000 3.0 0N.0. N00- 80. 80- 0.0- 000 .00 80 0N.0. 2.0. 0N.0. N00 0.0. 0.0 N..0 .040 0N.N 00.0 N00 N ..0- 8.0 v . .0 0.0 00.0 00.0 0N.0 N00 NN.0 0 ..0 00.0 0N.0 :0 N00- 0.0 .00 0.0 a 8.0 00.0. N00 00.0 0.0- 00.0 . ..0 20. N00 000 m ..0 0 ..0 00.0 00.0 0N.0 N00 N00. 80. N00 00.0 N00 .010 0. .N . 3.: 0 _.0 N00 N00 0 ..0 m ..0 . ..0- 0.0- 0.0 .00- 0 ..0 00.0 00.0 NN.0 80 30 m ..0 00.0- 00.0 000 00.0 .00. .0., 0. an... a... “:3. 88 N08 .08 N038 0880 .0008 N880 .880 .83 Na... .30.. 8008 .0008 N808 =58 2.8 N50: ...—0: .552 mmm 30). 00.0 mas—0.05000 .0.—5000M. .000 000.00.000000005 .wN 030... 153 0.80 080.0000 0208.300 n 00.0 0.80 00.0 003 80800000 n 0030 £080.00 30200 n 0000 £080,000 02002000 .8020 n 800 £030.00 0232000 00000200 n .000 m00.000 008620 N 80000 00.08 02030 M 000000 M00.000 03082 n .0000 080000 000.000 n 0000000 0.0000 02090 N 50000 0800 0080000800 M 00000 00.0000 .003.00 00 00.0 00.00—00.70 H .mE/ww ”003.30 000800 ..0 .00.0000< H Nmmmz .00.~§000.0 ..0 .00.0000< ".mmmz 0.000.000.0000 3.000203 .020000 N ~003me 3000006 35000000 5.000203 w50.>000 H .mbmmm 300003m 0000 $063000 H gmme 000:0 0000 32080 n .000 00 080000 0.0080 82800 N 20000 5.000000 00 00.002000 n 0200.). 0000000 00 00.000800 n .2082 000800000 803 u 2200.00 M00500.0 0.0 8 0.0000 000.0008 00.00.00 0.0100. 0&0 0.0 m... ...v 00.0N 0N.0N 00.0 00.2 N040 «0.0 0N.0- 00.0- a :0- 3.0- NI0I... 00.0 3.10 00.0. 3.0. 3.0- 3.0- 3.0 00.0 00.... 3.0 00.: 0N.0- 0N.0. 0. .0 00.0 30 a0 0. 3.0 .N.0. 00.0- .00- 00.0 3.0 50 N00 30 :0 00.NN 00.0 m _ .0 N00- 00.0- 20. N00- 000. 000 N10 0... . vN0 0N.0 vN0- 0.0- 0 . .0- .00 2.0.. 3.0 N000 00.0 0N.0N 0. .0 0N.0 0.0. N ..0- N ..0. .00. .00. $0 000 a 000 0N.0 2.0- 0.0. v. .0 0. .0 0.0 2.0 0N.0 N..0- v.0- 0 . .0- 0.010 N0.0 0. .0 _ v. .0- 00.0- 00.0 .N.0 0. .0 0. .0 ... .0 000 N00- 00.0 00.0 $0 3.0 0.0. .N.0- N..0. 00.0 :0 0.0 00.0 0.0 0.0 00.0- 00.0 3.0 00.0 a 3.0 0...... _ m0 Nm.0 00.0- mN0- 0N.0- 00.0- "..0. 00.0 0 . .0 0N.0 0 _ .0- N00- 20. 000 .0.... 00.N. 0N.0 0 _ .0 . ..0. 00.0. . ..0. 3.0 00.0. 0.0 00.0 0.0 00.0 000 N00. R0 00.... 000 No.0. 00.0 .00 m ..0- NN.0- 0N.0. N ..0. 0 . .0- NN.0 3.0 0M0 0.0.0- 3.0 3.0 000 . . .0 340 03 Nm.0N 0N.0 00.0 0 . .0- 0N.0- 0N.0- 0.0- 0 _ .0. N ..0 3.0 NN.0 00.0.. .00- 00.0 00.0 MNO 00.0 9040 00.0 m N. . v 3.0 :0 «N0. :0. 0N.0-. 0N.0- . m0- 0N.0 $0 0N.0 v _ .0- . 0.0- 00.0 0N.0 e _ .0 00.0 00.0 00.0 0 _ .N .0.... 00.0 0N.0. 00.0- . _ .0 00.0 00.0 3.0 2.0- m . .0 3.0 00.0 00.0 00.0 N00- 00.0 00.0- NN.0 N . .0 0. .0 v. .0 0N.0 0N.0 00.0 N. .0 3.0 00.0 0N.0 :0 8.0 .00. _ ..0 00.0 .00 0. .0 3.0- 00.0 m . .0 NN.0 a .00 0.01.0 00.0. 3.00 _ ..0 00.0 v0.0. «00 N00 00.0 00.0 m. .0 8.0 0N.0 0. .0 00.0 :0 8.0. 00.0 00.0 .00 0. .0 3.0 .00 0040 0%.. 00 :2: 0:0 0.30 0000 N000 .000 N03... 3080 .03... $080 _ 0080 . 25 N03: .000: $0000 .0300 Nan—00.. .53.. .23.. N50: . ...“: ::3 .oN 030,0 154 The relative fit of the model with its factor loadings and structural paths constrained to equality [x2 (df358, N=201) = 402.30, p > 0.001, GFI = 0.88, RMSEA = 0.04, CFI = 0.94, NNFI = 0.93, PNFI = 0.66, PGFI = 1.37] was not significantly worse (xzdm‘mnce test p = 0.67) than that of the unconstrained model [)8 (df 336, N=201) = 383.62, p < 0.001, GFI = 0.88, RMSEA = 0.04, CFI =0.94, NNFI =0.92, PNFI = 0.63, PGF I = 1.28]. Please see Table 23. Thus, we can conclude that the structural paths do not differ for the high and low SES groups and SES is not a moderator. lie—ritage Tables 30 and 31 display the intercorrelation matrices, means, and standard deviations used to test differences between mono-ethnic and bi-ethnic participants, respectively. The evaluation of measurement invariance revealed that the fit of the equality-constrained, factor loadings model for mono-ethnic (E=79) and bi-ethnic participants (3:122) [x2 (df 352, N=201) = 471.73, p < 0.001, GFI = 0.84, RMSEA = 0.06, CFI = 0.91, NNFI =0.89, PNFI = 0.63, PGFI = 1.28] was significantly worse (xzdmmnce test p < 0.05) than that of the unconstrained model [)8 (df 338, N=201) = 434.70, p < 0.001, GFI = 0.84, RMSEA = 0.05, CFI = 0.92, NNFI = 0.90, PNFI = 0.62, PGFI = 1.23]. Please see Table 23. Faced with these results, an analysis of the equality of the factor variances and covariances was conducted. Thus, the fit of a model where only error variances and covariances were estimated for each group [x2 (df 324, N=201) = 425.30, p < 0.001, GFI = 0.85, RMSEA = 0.06, CFI = 0.91, NNFI = 0.89, PNFI = 0.60, PGF I = 1.19] was compared to the preceding model where the factor loadings and error variances and covariances were constrained. The fit of the model with unconstrained error variances and covariances was significantly better (xzdmame test p < 0.05) than that 155 oiomwmofiaaum o>zoofzm H mIm ...oEom as 515 actoémnnm u "Skim Mmfioafixm 320:5 u mDmQ MmEOEEm ozmmoaoQ 3293“: u NQmO mmEoEEzm 0330509 _ncomuoEm H :DmD mwfimoo «Gavmojw u N009: mwfimoo 2,8an M mmmOUm mwaaoo 3883. u 882 €896 waxoom u ammoom Mmafia 953. u Encom amoam .885835 n 83% £856 3330 9 26 8:83.... u EB; 6:55 3950 co Began... u 332 aofiuficfio mo 38%.... u: 39.: uoSSfiom 55.353 Uozoood n NmDmmm ”ton—mam _oncoflom hump—02:3 w52>05 n EDmmm 3.5.5.5 coon w52>0um u ”Emma: 5&5 bug 838% u 7&me €258 baa..— Bzooom u :52 3.29% :o eocegaxm 1 S252 .mzoifim .«o aocawonvom n 32:34 “counacoto 95.5 H ~<2md< «Enema? 05 no Boa—ma mafia—bog bani—om .802 3.3 3.: 2.:: ::.:: a. E. 2.. 3.3 ~:.: 3.: ::.:. 2.:: 3.? 3.:- 3.? 3.? 3.? 3.? 3.:: 3.: ::.: g 3.: .31: 3.: ~33 2.? 2.:. 2.: 3.: 3.: ~13 ::.: 3.2 3.:. :~.? 2.: 3.: ::.: 3.: ma ::.: ::.: 3.:. 3.? 2.: 3.: 2.: 2.: ~_.? al.: >. 2.: 2.:. ::.:. _~.: ::.: ::.:- 2.: 2.: ::.: 3.: 3.: :3 3.2 ::.: _ _.: ::.:. ::.: ::.: ::.: ::.: ::.: ::.: Si: 3.... ::.: _ _.: :_.:~ 2.:. 2.:. 2.? 2.:. 3.: 2.: 2.: 5.: _ ..: 2.: 2.: 2.: m~.: ::.: 3.: 2.: ::.:. ~:.: ~:.: 2.: a 3.: g ::.:. ::.:. 3.? 2.? ::.: 2.: 2.: 3.: 2.: 2.: 3.: . _.: ::.: ::.: a ::.v ::.:. 2.: 2.: ::.: ::.:. 2.? _ _.:- ::.:. 3.: 2.: 2.: ::.:- ::.: 3.:- an: 3.: ::.: ::.:. 3.? 2.: ~:.: 3.: 2.: 2.: 3.: ::.: 3.: _~.: :— .: 2 .: ::.: ad: 2.: 3.:. 2.: ::.: ::.:. ::.: ::.:- ::.: «_.:- ::.: 2.: 2.: 2.: 2.: ::.: _~.: 3.: 94: 2.: 2.:: 3.: 2.: 3.:. 2.: ::.: :~.: 3.: ::.: ::.: 2.: 2.: 2.: 2.: 2.: ::.: ::.: fl 3.: 3.:: 2.: 3.: 2.:. 2.:. ::.:. 2.: ::.:. 2.: 3.: 3.: _ _.:- ::.:. ::.:. 2.: ::.: 2.: -.: .13 ::.~ ::.: ::.:- ::.:. 3.: 2.: ::.: 2.: ::.: 3.: ::.:. ::.: :~.: ::.: 2.: ::.: ::.: ::.: m:.: 2.: .31.: 2.: ::.:N ::.: ::.: 2.? 3.: ::.: ::.:. 2.: v _ .: ::.: :~.: ::.: Q: 2.: 2.: 2.: ::.: 3.:. «_.: :m.: a 2.:. 2.3 2.: ::.: 2.? 2.: 3.: ::.:. ~_.? :~.: 2.? 2.: ::.: 2.: 2.: 2.: :~.: 3.: 3.: 2.: ::.: 3.: 3.: a :3: :5 :3: 33 33 _ :3 33: 3.3:: 53: ~38: _ :8: _ :3 Ga: :3: Ear. :32 ~53: :58 35.. ::.:: _ t. a: 3:93 156 28m mmosaaam 2:833 u $5 638 0.3 5.3 5:823 n ”Sam gagim 3203 u 38 ”8.892% 8.3280 3235 n 88 Mmafiasam 058.39 acacoem u 58 W330 “529:... u 80me $580 9.93 u 8.50m mwsaou 9832 u 68%. 68:3 mfioom u 8.58 mwsaou 2.8.4. u 5.58 .365 .285825 u 8.6% £25m .8830 2 26 8.38.2 n 5.2m 6330 8950.... Hana? n «3%: accgoaa «o 5.2%? u. 3%: “.2888."— .fi.m3>.:5 .5383: u Ngmmm ”tonasm .2535“: 366.65 wEvSEL H :3me mtoaasm 50m 9:365 H gmmmm 5&3 30.. 8%8m u ::me €253 $5... 8383. u 2.53 568%. Mo coaeoaxm n ~25: 3225.5 :0 coacwooom H 223:2 603858 058.: n 3329533 ”Enema? 05 :0 Roman 385508 35233: .802 3.3 3.: 2.. 3.3 3.: 3.: 3.: 3.: ::.: 3.3 3.: 3... :33 3.3 ::.: 3.: ::.: 3.: m~.~ ....m ::.: ...: ~m.:~ ~3.3~ :3.m ::.:. 3.:. 3.3 3.:. :..- 3:... 2.3 2.... 3.:. 2.:. .2. :3.~. 3.:. 3.3 ::.3 3.: 3.:. 3.3 so: mad ::.: 3.? 3.? 3.? 3.? ::.:- 2.: 3.: :..: 2.? :~.? 3.:. 3.: .~.: 3.: 3.: 3.: 3.:. ::.: ~..: 3...: an: 3.? :3? ::.:- 3.? 33.? 3.: _~.: 3.: 3.? 2.? 2.:- ::.: 3.: :~.: 3.: 3.: 3.:- ::.: 3.: :3: a1: 3.: 3.: 2.: 3.: 3.? 3.:- 3.:- 3.: 3..: 3..: 3.:. :..? ::.:. 3.? ::.:. ::.: 2.? 3.: 83 a: 3.: 33.: 3.: 2.? ::.:- ~. .:- :. .: 3.: 3.: 3.? 2.? : . .? 3.? 3.? m. .: ::.: 3.: 33 a: :3: ::.: :~.? ~..:- ~. .:- 3.: o. .: 3. .: 3.? 2.:. 2.? .~.? :~.? ::.: 3.: ::.: .:3 a ::.: . _.? ::.: 3.? 3.: :. .: a. .: :. .:- ::.:- 3.? : . .? 3.? 3.:- ::.:- 3.? 38... :43 m..? :~.? 3.:- 3.: 3.: m. .: :. .:- 2.? : . .? o . .? 3.? ::.:- 3.? 3.? 3:8: 3.: 33.: ::.: 3. .? 3.:- 3.: 3.: :~.: 3.: : . .: 3.: m. .: :_ .: : _ .: 33... 34: 3a.: :. .? ::.:- ~. .? m. .: 3.: 3.: 3.: 3.: ... .: .~.: ~..: ~38: 3.... 3.? 3.: 3.? :~.: ::.: 3.: : . .: o . .: :~.: -.: : . .: .38: 3...... 3.: ::.: .~.? 3.? 3 . .? : . .? 3.? 3.:- 3.: 3.? $3: 3.: 3.: :. .? ::.:- 3.? : . .? 3.:- 3.: ::.: 3.: 3a: :34: :. .:- 3. .? ::.: 3. .? ::.:- m. .: 3.: 3. .: .3: mad ::.: 3.: 3.: :..: 3.? 3.:. ::.: 33:. an: 2.: .~.: ::.: ::.:- 3.? ::.:- .33.. and 3.: 3.: 2.: :..: 2.: 3:2 a: 3.: 3.? 2.: 3.? .52 3...: . _.: 3.: .~.: .35.. a 3.: 3.: ~:.:: a 3.: .:m: 31: 2:2 ::.: :3: 38 38 .3: 33.: 3:8: 33... 3:8: .38: E3: 33: 5:: ~32 .32 3.3.. .52 .52 ~:.:: .:m: ::3 .:m 03.3. 157 of the unconstrained factor loadings model. This result suggested that it was really the variances and covariances that differed between groups. To determine which variances and covariances were contributing to this overall inequality, the equality of each element in the Phi matrix was tested independently. Following the procedure suggested by Byrne (1998), a model was specified in which parameters of the Phi matrix (i.e., (Du; variance of Individual Cultural Characteristics) were cumulatively constrained across groups. The fit of the resulting model was then compared to the model in which only the factor loadings were constrained equal across groups. If the xzdifi‘erenoe test revealed that the two models were significantly different, the constrained Phi parameter was allowed to be estimated in the subsequent model. However, if the {dim-mm test was not significantly different, the Phi parameter was held equal across groups in the subsequent model. All elements in the Phi matrix were tested in this manner and equality constraints were maintained only for parameters found to be equal for both groups. These tests of the Phi matrix revealed that three variances (e.g., Individual Cultural Characteristics, Appraisal of Sociocultural Stress, Distress) and three covariances (e. g., Perceived Social Support and Wellbeing, Appraisal of Sociocultural Stress and Indirect Coping, Appraisal of Sociocultural Stress and Wellbeing) were nonequivalent across groups. The fit of the model in which these variances and covariances were freely estimated for both groups [)8 (df 346, N=201) = 442.77, p < 0.001, GFI = 0.84, RMSEA = 0.05, CFI = 0.92, NNFI = 0.90, PNFI = 0.63, PGFI = 1.26] was then compared to the above model in which factor loadings and error variances and covariances were estimated for both groups. The xzmemce test for these two models was not significant (p = 0.43). Please see Table 23. As a result, we can assume that these six variances and covariances are what differ across 158 groups and not the factor loadings. Therefore, the indicators are believed to measure the latent variables in comparable ways for mono-ethnic and bi-ethnic participants. A test of group differences in the means of the latent constructs revealed that compared to mono-ethnic participants, bi-ethnic participants exhibited significantly higher levels on Indirect Coping (x = 1.18, t = 2.23, p <0.05, D = -0.45) as well as lower levels on Individual Cultural Characteristics (x = -7.46, t = -5.40, p < 0.001, D = 0.93) and Appraisal of Sociocultural Stress (K = -3.03, t = -2.28, p < 0.05, D = 0.46). Statistically significant differences were not evident on Perceived Social Support (x = 1.67, t_ = 1.96, p = 0.05, D = -0.39) Direct Coping (x = -0.73, t = -l.00, p > 0.10, D = 0.20), Distress (K = -0.81, t = -l.11, p > 0.20, D = 0.28), Wellbeing (K = -O.57, t = -1.23, 9 >020, D = 0.24). The relative fit of the model with its factor loadings and structural paths constrained to equality [)8 (df 358, N=201) = 483.05, p < 0.001, GFI = 0.83, RMSEA = 0.06, CFI = 0.91, NNFI = 0.89, PNFI = 0.62, PGFI = 1.22] was found to be significantly worse (xzdiffmme test p < 0.001) than that of the unconstrained model [352 (df 336, N=201) = 438.17, p < 0.001, GFI = 0.84, RMSEA = 0.06, CFI = 0.91, NNFI = 0.89, PNFI = 0.62, PGFI = 1.22]. Please see Table 23. Thus it can be concluded that the structural paths (relationships among the latent variables) differ for bi-ethnic and mono-ethnic participants. More specifically, the path from Individual Cultural Characteristics to Direct Coping (standardized B = 0.16, _t_ = 1.62, p > 0.10, two-tailed test; Figure 14) was not statistically significant for bi-ethnic participants. This same path (Individual Cultural Characteristics to Direct Coping; standardized B = 0.06, t = 0.22, p > 0.20, two-tailed test; 159 Figure 15) in addition to the paths from Direct Coping to Wellbeing (standardized B = 0.14, t = 1.34, p > 0.10, two-tailed test; Figure 15) Perceived Social Support to both Direct Coping (standardized B = 0.85, t = 1.61, p_ > 0.10, two-tailed test; Figure 15) and Indirect Coping (standardized B = -0.07, _t = -0.33, p > 0.20, two-tailed test; Figure 15) were also not significant for mono-ethnic participants. Three statistically significant indirect relationships were found for mono-ethnic and bi-ethnic participants. Indirect relationships between Appraisal of Sociocultural Stress to both Distress (mono-ethnic: n = 0.25, t = 2.56, p < 0.02; bi-ethnic: n = 0.21, t = 2.24, p <05) and Wellbeing (mono-ethnic: n = -O.15, t = -2.33, p <.02; bi-ethnic: n = - 0.16, t = -2.14, p <05) and Indirect Coping to Wellbeing (mono-ethnic: n = -0.41, t = - 2.56, p <.02; bi-ethnic: n = -0.64, t = -3.15, p < 0.01) were also found. Thus, mono- ethnic and bi-ethnic participants who experienced sociocultural stress utilized indirect coping strategies, experienced higher levels of distress and lower levels of wellbeing. 160 253. 2835 053m .8 38.2 Begum .3 3%: 2a u €65 .35 53:; see 868$ .3” ".6 .2 .3. u "x :98: Ragga: «Sum—2 8.2 ... E65 .35 8.3:; :20 38$ .84 new .848” u "x H.252 :32 new“ , 3:. :25: 1.530 to 36 363%.: 223 «to "Em 89am . «mmmz 533838 E Sq co E§&< :3 8mm: «a... 7.5mm“— "I. _3 «gamma _