a , 1"l t". I hv‘ 5M1 .‘ -¢;.-. ..... mg-‘wsammm: Eu w w" _' ': ii “a”... are??? ,4 A N «“341»?! ,5: a: ' [‘67 ’5! ~5KLZM. nun“... . ... “an ~I'u "' , ". 3332* (.,. . nu... . ‘5, 3 43 1 I: Eff $9.23; ‘ l :- a ‘ THEIS 515%?” This is to certify that the dissertation entitled PARENTAL EFFICACY, ATTACHMENT, AND CHILD SELF-CONTROL: A COMPARISON OF PROCESSES AMONG DEMOGRAPHIC GROUPS presented by M. ANGELA NIEVAR has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for the Doctoral degree in Family and Child Ecology - . /" i%azm M?‘ Major Profeésgr's Signature 51/9139! Date MSU is an Affimiative Action/Equal Opportunity Institution 'UBRARY I Michigan State University PLACE IN RETURN BOX to remove this checkout from your record. TO AVOID FINES return on or before date due. MAY BE RECALLED with earlier due date if requested. DATE DUE DATE DUE DAIEURUE JAN 110 2096; “IL?“ 1‘ L» w FEB‘IZLSH 200$ 6/01 cJCIRC/DateDuo.p65—p. 15 PARENTAL EFFICACY, ATTACHMENT, AND CHILD SELF-CONTROL: A COMPARISON OF PROCESSES AMONG DEMOGRAPHIC GROUPS By M. Angela Nievar A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fitlfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Family and Child Ecology 2004 ABSTRACT PARENTAL EFFICACY, ATTACHMENT, AND CHILD SELF -CONTROL: A COMPARISON OF PROCESSES AMONG DEMOGRAPHIC GROUPS By M. Angela Nievar According to Bandura’s (1982) self-efficacy theory, parents who feel competent and effective, or who have high levels of parental efficacy, are more likely to invest time and energy in parenting. In fact, parental efiicacy appears to be related to parental resources and practices; fewer studies have examined the relation between parental efficacy and children’s development (Bandura, Barbaranelli, Caprara, & Pastorelli, 2001; Brazelton, 1983; Bugental & Shenum, 1984; Teti & Gelfand, 1991). This dissertation examined linkages between parental efficacy, parenting, attachment security, and child self-control. The effects of parental efficacy and parenting may be different in various environments. Bronfenbrenner (2000) theorized that the effects of parenting are dependent on risks inherent within a child’s environment, such as poverty or racism. In fact, research indicates that effects of parental efficacy vary by socioeconomic status, race, and marital quality (Ardelt & Eccles, 2001; Corapci & Wachs, 2002; Machida, Taylor, & Kim, 2002). Using data from the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (N ICHD) Early Child Care Research Network, this dissertation empirically described family processes related to parental efficacy within low-income and middle-income families and within African American and Caucasian families. Structural equation models indicated that parental efficacy and parental practices are predictive of attachment security and children’s self-control. Linkages between parenting and child outcomes were generally stronger among low-income groups than middle-income groups, suggesting that proximal processes have a greater effect among children living in disadvantaged families than among children with more resources. Distinct effects were found within African American and Caucasian groups. Different dimensions of parental efficacy, perceived competence and perceived control, produced different effects on parenting style and child outcomes within demographic groups. Marital conflict appeared to have a greater effect on boys than girls; other gender differences raised interesting questions that warrant further investigation. In sum, this dissertation added to our understanding of parental efficacy and its relation to various aspects of children’s environments, attachment security, and child self-control within diverse demographic groups. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS I would like to acknowledge the assistance I have received from the Graduate School, the Department of Family and Child Ecology, and the professors and students that have furthered my work. I would like to thank Dr. Luster for his assistance and advice throughout my graduate program as my major professor, particularly in helping me through the publication process on multiple occasions. I am also grateful to Dr. Luster for submitting my admissions application to the University Distinguished Fellowship program. I would like to thank the Graduate School for supporting me in that program throughout my four years at Michigan State University. I would like to thank Dr. Holly Brophy-Herb and Dr. Hiram Fitzgerald for their support and their encouragement in my work as a research assistant. I am grateful for the travel support from Dr. Hiram Fitzgerald, Dr. Holly Brophy-Herb, the Graduate School, the college, and the department. I am also thankful for research support received from the Society for the Psychological Study of Social Issues, the Graduate School, the College of Human Ecology, and the Department of Family and Child Ecology. I want to thank Dr. Anne Soderman for her support and encouragement in my research endeavors. I would like to thank Dr. Marguerite Barratt for her encouragement and assistance in obtaining data for the NICHD Early Child Care Study. I am grateful to Dr. Deborah Johnson for her insight on that study. I am also grateful to the numerous researchers and assistants that developed the study, collected the data, and completed previous works, providing a foundation for this dissertation. iv Finally, I would like to thank my family. I am grateful to my parents for providing a safe haven and fiee child care. I am thankful to them for giving me an enriched home environment that allowed me to develop a love of learning. I am grateful to my husband for his patience, assistance, and support. Finally, I would like to thank my children for just being there and being wonderful. TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES ............................................................................... viii LIST OF FIGURES .............................................................................. x CHAPTER 1 INTRODUCTION ............................................................................... 1 Purpose of the Study ........................................................................ 3 General Research Questions ............................................................... 5 Attachment in Infancy .............................................................. 5 Social-emotional Development in Early Chilrfltood ............................ 5 Research Assumptions ....................................................................... 8 Importance of Parenting ........................................................... 8 Influence of Parental Beliefs ...................................................... 8 Ecological Perspective ............................................................ 9 Justification of the Research .............................................................. 9 Overview of the Dissertation ............................................................. 13 CHAPTER 2 LITERATURE REVIEW ...................................................................... 15 Ecological Theory ........................................................................... l 5 Social Cognitive Theory .................................................................. 18 Parental Eflicacy .................................................................. 20 Diversity and Parental Efl‘icacy ................................................. 22 Attachment Theory ......................................................................... 24 Correlates of A ttachment ........................................................ 26 Conclusion ................................................................................... 41 CHAPTER 3 METHOD ........................................................................................ 42 Hypotheses .................................................................................. 42 Dimensions of Parental Efficacy and Attachment in Infancy ............... 42 Ecological Model .................................................................. 45 Sample ....................................................................................... 46 Attrition ............................................................................. 51 Measurement ................................................................................. 52 Parental Characteristics .......................................................... 55 Contextual Characteristics ........................................................ 57 Data Analysis ............................................................................... 62 vi CHAPTER 4 RESULTS ....................................................................................... 63 Preliminary Analyses ....................................................................... 63 Univariate Analyses ............................................................... 63 Bivariate Analyses ................................................................ 65 Tests of Hypotheses ......................................................................... 86 Conclusion ................................................................................... l 14 CHAPTER 5 DISCUSSION .................................................................................... l 15 Research Questions .......................................................................... 115 Dimensions of Parental Efficacy ................................................. l 16 Child Care Quality .................................................................. 123 Social-Emotional Development in Early Childhood......... .. . . .. 124 Marital Conflict ...................................................................... 126 Gender Diyfirences ................................................................ 129 Limitations ..................................................................................... 131 Future Directions ............................................................................ 133 Conclusion ...................................................................................... 135 APPENDICES .................................................................................... 137 REFERENCES ................................................................................... 148 vii LIST OF TABLES 1 Income-to-needs ratios by time of data collection ....................................... 48 2 Descriptive statistics of study variables ................................................... 50 3 Measures, constructs, and age of child at administration ....................................... 53 4 Frequencies of dichotomous study variables by demographic groups ............... 67 5 Correlations between study variables for the total sample ............................. 69 6 Correlations for African American and Caucasian families ........................... 70 7 Correlations for low-income and middle-to-high income families ..................... 71 8 Correlations for study variables by child gender ........................................ 72 9 Tests for equality of means and equality of variances between African American and Caucasian groups ....................................................................... 74 10 Study variables by economic group ........................................................ 76 l I Study variables by gender ................................................................... 78 12 Means of study variables by attachment classification with one-way analyses of variance ..................................................................................... 81 13 Study variables by attachment security .................................................... 82 14 Hypotheses and tests related to perceived control ....................................... 86 15 Hypotheses and tests of perceived competence ........................................... 89 16 Parenting measures and strange situation classification with demographic groups .......................................................................................... 92 17 Multinomial logistic regression model of attachment classification for the total sample .......................................................................................... 94 18 Multinomial logistic regression model for Afiican American families ................ 95 19 Hypotheses and tests of moderators of child care quality ............................... 96 20 Hypotheses and corresponding significance tests of variable-level paths ........... 100 viii 21 Path coefl‘icients and estimates by child gender .......................................... 104 22 Tests for equality of means and equality of variances between low-income Afiican American and Caucasian groups .................................................. 144 23 Tests for equality of means and equality of variances between midrfle-to-high income Afiican American and Caucasian groups ........................................ 146 ix LIST OF FIGURES Hypothesized model of interactions with child care quality predicting attachment security ........................................................................... 6 Hypothesized model of pathways leading to child outcomes: self-control and attachment security ....................................................................... 7 Hypothesized model of perceived competence, responsive parenting, and attachment classifications .................................................................... 44 Structural equation model with predictors of attachment and self-control ............ 102 Structural equation model with predictors of attachment and self-control, excluding single-parent families ............................................................ 107 Structural equation model with low-income families .................................... 109 Structural equation model with low-income families, excluding single-parent families ......................................................................................... 110 Structural equation model with middle-to-high income families ....................... 112 Structural equation model with middle-to-high income families, excluding single-parent families ........................................................................ 113 CHAPTER 1 INTRODUCTION The type of environment that parents provide for their children may be influenced by their beliefs. Some parents may believe that they are efi‘ective teachers for their children, although others do not. Their perception of their own effectiveness is defined as parental efficacy (Bandura, 1982; Coleman & Karraker, 1997). Theoretically, parents who feel that they are important and effective in their role will spend more time and effort with their children. Research indicates that parental efficacy is related to parental resources and practices, such as poverty, maternal depression, and harsh discipline, and child characteristics such as temperament and attachment (Brazelton, 1983; Bugental & Shenum, 1984; Nievar & Brophy-Herb, 2003; Luster & Kain, 1987; Teti & Gelfand, 1991). Experimental studies aimed at improving the home environment for young children through parent education show related improvements among parental efficacy, parenting, and child outcome (Hastings & Brown, 2002; Hoath & Sanders, 2002; Sofronoff & Farbotko, 2002). The effects of parental efficacy and parenting may be different in various environments. Bronfenbrenner (2000) theorized that the effects of parenting are dependent on risks inherent within a child’s environment, such as poverty or racism. In fact, research indicates that effects of parental efficacy vary by socioeconomic status, race, and marital quality (Ardelt & Eccles, 2001; Corapci & Wachs, 2002; Machida, Taylor, & Kim, 2002). Developmental processes related to parental practices may also vary due to cultural differences (Johnson et al., 2003; McLoyd, Cauce, Takeuchi, & Wilson, 2000). Cultural requirements may affect the way that parents think about parenting, and in turn affect parenting practices (Ogbu, 1985). For example, certain cultures emphasize children’s self-control more than others. Thus, parents in these cultures may provide more opportunities for a child to learn to regulate their emotions. Although ways of parenting may differ by culture, processes leading to child outcome may be more universal. For example, power-assertive discipline appears to inhibit the development of secure attachment and self-control (Davies, Harold, Goeke- Morey, & Cummings, 2002; Kochanska & Knaack, 2003). Children’s attachment security is related to their self-control as well as to the type of parenting they receive (Londerville & Main, 1981; Braungart-Rieker, Garwood, Powers, & Wang, 2001; Cummings & Davies, 1996; Cassidy, 1994)TI€PaCIr)'ents who are insensitive and unresponsive to their child’s signals are more likely to have children who have difficulty controlling their emotions or behavior (Rothbart & Bates, 1998). Parents who are insensitive and unresponsive are also less likely to have children who develop secure attachment relationships (De Wolff & van IJzendoorn, 1997). Although relations between sensitivity, attachment, and self-control have been researched, the study of parental efficacy and attachment is a relatively new area. A search of the literature revealed no studies of parental efficacy and children’s self-control. Literature that exists on parental efficacy, parenting, and attachment reports inconsistent findings, particularly in the area of perceived parental competence (del Carmen, Pedersen, Huffman & Bryan, 1993; Spieker & Booth, 1988; Teti & Gelfand, 1991). Discrepancies in the literature may be the result of a misconception that a strong sense of competence is always preferred. Some experimental work suggests that overly confident parents who have a strong sense of competence may not be as sensitive as those who have a more moderate perception of their abilities (Donovan & Leavitt, 1989; Donovan, Leavitt, & Walsh, 1997; Donovan, Leavitt, & Walsh, 2000). Descriptive studies indicate that clinically depressed mothers who have a weak sense of competence are more likely to have problems with parenting their children effectively (T eti, O’Connell, & Reiner, 1996). Thus, high levels of perceived competence and low levels of perceived competence may both be related to inadequate parenting; however, most measm'es are based on a linear scale that assumes high levels of competence to be optimal. A second explanation for discrepant findings may be the way that parental eflicacy is conceptualized. Parental efficacy is composed of multiple dimensions, including perceived control over situations and perception of personal competence. Perceived competence and perceived control may represent two separate belief systems that have different effects on parenting and child outcomes. Purpose of the Study This dissertation focused on parental efficacy and its relation to parenting and mother-child attachment, particularly among low-income families. Separate analyses investigated the process of attachment within two periods of development: (a) in infancy, and (b) in early childhood. Correspondence between parental efficacy, family processes in infancy, and attachment classification at 15 months was analyzed. Pathways between study variables, attachment security at 24 months, and children’s self-control in early childhood were also examined. An examination of attachment quality at 15 months tests the hypothesis that parental efficacy is related to parenting and attachment in predictable ways. Because parental beliefs vary among demographic groups, dimensions of parental efficacy were studied among low-income and middle-to-high income families and African American and Caucasian families. Separate analyses for other ethnic or racial groups are not included due to the limited number of Latinos, Native Americans, and Asians in the study. Second, a longitudinal model was used to test the pathways between parental efficacy in infancy, parenting behavior, children’s attachment security at 24 months, and children’s self-control in early childhood. Other factors that may affect child outcome, such as marital conflict, are taken into account. Child outcome is measured by the security of children’s attachment to their mother and performance on a self-control task. Models compare processes between low-income and high-to-middle income families only. The use of structural equation modeling requires a minimum sample size of at least 150 families. Although the NICHD Study of Early Child Care began with 176 African American families, there were only 119 African American children who participated in the procedure measuring child self-control at 36 months. This sample size does not allow for separate models comparing developmental processes as stated above between African American and Caucasian children. General Research Questions Attachment in Infancy I. Can counterintuitive findings from earlier studies of the relation between parental efficacy and attachment security be explained by reexamining dimensions of parental efficacy, i.e., competence and control? 2. Does parental efficacy have different effects on parenting and attachment among different demographic groups, specifically, low-income and middle-to-high income families and African American and Caucasian families? 3. Does child care quality moderate the effects of maternal depression and parenting practices on mother-child attachment as shown in Figure 1? Social-emotional Development in Early Childhood 4. Are the data fiom the National Institute of Child Health and Development Study of Early Child Care consistent with the model linking parental efficacy, parenting practice, and children’s social-emotional outcomes as shown in Figure 2? 5. Does the model fit vary as a function of demographic status? For example, do the relations among the variables differ for low-income and middle-to-high income families? Positive Parenting Child Care Quality Attachment Security Maternal Depression Figure I . Hypothesized model of interactions with child care quality predicting attachment security. Attachment _ --------- Security CUIMU ": \ Racism PM” m; Child '~ " Self-Control Child Tern Figure 2. Hypothesized model of pathways leading to child outcomes: self-control and attachment security. Research Assumptions Importance of Parenting Parenting practices are central to children’s developing competencies in many different areas (Bomstein, 2002). In recent years, there has been some debate over the relative importance of parenting practices in comparison to inborn traits and peer influences (Harris, 2002; Scarr, 1998). Some child characteristics, such as various dimensions of temperament, have shown heritability; however, secure attachment to parental figures, an important factor in early personality development, has not shown significant heritability (Vaughn & Best, 1999). Thus, in the development of mother- child attachment, nurture or parenting is more essential than genetic endowment. Influence of Parental Beliefs In addition to parenting practices, it is also important to consider parental beliefs about parenting. There has been some debate concerning differences between beliefs and behavior. In both psychology and anthropology, some have argued that the relationship between beliefs and behavior is difficult to detect (Harkness & Super, 1992). A relation between cultural group values and beliefs is not disputed; however, individual variation in practice within cultures may result from either selective attention to societal norms, personal practices that differ from publicly expressed values, or outright nonconforrnist behavior (Gjerde, 2001; Grusec, Hastings, & Mammone, 1994). However, the assumption that parenting practices are influenced by beliefs about what constitutes acceptable behavior is generally upheld (Sigel, McGillicuddy—DeLisi, & Goodnow, 1992). It is of particular interest that varying parental beliefs within cultural groups often explain differences in parenting behavior, which may, in turn, improve the prediction of children’s developmental outcomes (Harkness & Super, 1992). Ecological Perspective Other factors besides parenting also influence children’s development. Systems theory suggests that children are affected by multiple experiences and settings, directly and indirectly. Emotional regulation, fathering, marital conflict, and the physical environment have been associated with children’s attachment behavior in recent empirical research (Braungart-Rieker, Garwood, Powers, & Wang, 2001; Davies, Harold, Goeke-Morey, & Cummings, 2002; Diener, Nievar, & Wright, 2003). From a broader perspective, the National Research Council’s report on early childhood noted that multiple factors—including parents’ psychological adjustment, marital relationships, the child care environment, neighborhood, and culture--affect parenting, parent-child interactions, and child development (Shonkofi’ & Phillips, 2000). Justification of the Research The relation between parenting behavior, the parent-child attachment relationship, and child behavior has been previously researched. A meta-analysis of 66 studies from diverse cultures indicated that maternal behavior styles influence the quality of parent- child attachment relationships (De Wolff & van IJzendoorn, 1997). The quality of the attachment relationship, in turn, is related to children’s emotional regulation, an important factor in developing social skills (Calkins, 1994). Although parenting behavior, attachment, and children’s self-control have been researched, fewer studies have looked at how parental beliefs, such as parental efficacy, relate to child outcomes. Among studies relating parental efficacy to attachment, results are inconsistent (e.g., del Carmen, Pedersen, Huffman, & Bryan, 1993; Spieker & Booth, 1988; Teti & Gelfand, 1991). High parental efficacy is related to several parenting activities, ranging from improving parenting skills through self-education (Spoth & Conroy, 1993) to teaching children injury protection (Peterson, Farmer, & Kashani, 1990); however, some studies of parental efficacy and parenting behavior have produced unexpected results and noted confounding effects (del Carmen, Pedersen, Huffman & Bryan, 1993; Luster & Rhoades, 1989; Mash & Johnston, 1983; Spieker & Booth, 1988). An overly strong perception of self-competence and a weak sense of self-competence may both be related to suboptimal parenting behavior. However, a curvilinear relation between perceived competence and parenting behavior has not been specifically tested. Bandura (1982, 1986, 1994) has led the field in investigations of efficacy. His early work acknowledged multiple facets of self-efficacy beliefs, differentiating between perceived competence and control over outcomes, while acknowledging that they were interwoven (Bandura, 1982; Ozer & Bandura, 1990). Applying these principles to the domain of parenting, a self-perceived efficacious parent would need to believe: (a) in their personal competence and knowledge about parenting, and (b) that parenting makes a positive difference in children’s outcomes. Differentiation between perceived competence and perceived control might also clarify inconsistent findings in the research literature. 10 This dissertation investigates the mediators and moderators of the theoretical process represented pictorially in Figure 2. Distal factors, such as the quality of marital relationships, may affect attachment and children’s subsequent adjustment, through pathways such as parenting behavior (Davies, Harold, Goeke-Morey, & Cummings, 2002). Other research groups have investigated the development of parental efficacy, attachment, and child behavior; however, typically such research has used smaller samples and focused on a small number of variables. Hence, fewer studies examine interactions and mediational processes. A notable exception is Teti and colleagues’ work with high-risk families. In this series of studies, spousal support and child temperament moderated the relation between parent-child characteristics and parental efficacy, and parental efficacy mediated the relation between maternal depression and sensitivity (Teti, O’Connell, & Reiner, 1996). A model using a larger and more heterogeneous group of participants may extend these findings, allowing for the examination of processes among low-risk as well as high-risk groups. Additional research is needed to further define parental efficacy and explore its relation to parenting practice and child outcomes. In particular, research is necessary to untangle differences in developmental processes across diverse groups (Bronfenbrenner, 1999). What is normative in one cultural group or sub-group may not be appropriate in another. Cultures differ in some of their core beliefs, making certain types of parenting beliefs and behavior and certain types of child behavior more acceptable in some groups than in others. A scarcity of resources may also make certain types of behavior more acceptable or useful; some researchers have theorized that certain parenting behavior, 11 such as strict discipline, is adaptive for parents and children in African American families (Deater-Deckard, Dodge, Bates, & Pettit 1996; Gonzales, Cauce, Friedman, & Mason, 1996). Alternatively, this effect may be accounted for by socioeconomic status (Polaha, 1999). Yet other processes in child development are theorized to be constant across cultures or settings. In order to investigate diverse perspectives in parenting, it is important to have a sample that is similar to the population of interest. Although some research has been done on parenting processes among African Americans in the United States, most of this research has been among low-income inner-city families involved in intervention programs (Mcond, Cauce, Takeuchi, & Wilson, 2000). Although studies of prevention programs can teach us much about a subgroup of the population, it is important to recognize that an over-generalization of such research may create confusion. Research on single-parent African American families has been particularly emphasized in the last decade, with two-parent families simply used as a comparison. Furthermore, studies of parenting in Caucasian families often focus on middle-class samples that are also unrepresentative. The sample of participants in this dissertation reflects the population to a greater extent than studies using local convenience samples. Specifically, data were collected from families who participated in the NICHD Study of Child Care. The demographics of these participants reflect national income levels and racial proportions (N ICHD, 1999a). Thus, descriptions of family processes should be more representative of American families than most previous work. 12 In addition, varied environmental conditions may create circumstances wherein certain parenting practices are important for children’s well-being. Given that a family’s available resources influence child development (Shonkoff & Phillips, 2000), this dissertation will examine both low-income and middle-to-high income families. Given that racism and ethnic differences in parenting behavior may influence family processes (McLoyd, Cauce, Takeuchi, & Wilson, 2000), a model evaluating the influence of parental efficacy on attachment security will contrast African American and Caucasian groups. An ecological perspective assumes that factors such as ethnicity, neighborhood quality, and marital quality may influence child characteristics (Bronfenbrenner, 1992). Thus, distal factors, such as family income, and proximal factors, such as parenting practices and child characteristics, may alter the strength of the relation between parental efficacy and child outcome. In sum, this research will add to our current understanding of attachment relationships within diverse environments. Its emphasis on parental efficacy may explain the motivation behind parents’ actions and the connection of parents’ oognitions to parenting practice and child behavior. From an applied perspective, findings may assist in the development of culturally appropriate parent education curriculum or home visiting programs. Overview of the Dissertation Chapter 1 has presented a brief description of the research questions and the significance of this research. The following chapter presents a literature review describing the theoretical and empirical basis for this research. A discussion of 13 ecological theory provides a framework for understanding social cognitive theory and attachment theory. Applications of these theories as found in the empirical research literature are discussed. Group differences and similarities are also reviewed. Chapter 3 presents the methods and measures used to test the research questions described in Chapter 1. Chapter 4 presents empirical results, including frequencies, correlations, analyses of variance, logistic regressions, and structural equation models. Finally, Chapter 5 discusses these results, describes limitations, and suggests future research directions. 14 CHAPTER 2 LITERATURE REVIEW This dissertation draws on three areas of social science literature as a basis for its model: (a) ecological theory, (b) social cognitive theory, and (c) attachment theory. A description of these three theories and pertinent literature is followed by a review of literature on the correlates of attachment, including characteristics of the individual child, the home environment, the child care enviromnent, and distal factors. Literature discussing the process of children’s social-emotional development within diverse cultures is also presented. Ecological Theory Bronfenbrenner’s (1979) early ecological model focused on the environment and its effect on the developing individual. Historically, psychologists in the I9608 and 19703 spent more time in laboratory settings than in natural environments. In fact, clinical and research psychologists tended to discount effects of the total family, working conditions, and neighborhoods, focusing on individual perceptions apart from their environment. In contrast, Bronfenbrenner’s ecological model emphasized total environmental effects. Bronfenbrenner designated various levels of a child’s total environment as interactive systems, including the microsystem, mesosystem, exosystem, and macrosystem (Hamilton & Luster, 2003). The system likely to have the greatest effect on 15 a child was named the microsystem. By definition, the microsystem included the individual child. The home environment is a microsystem with parents, siblings, toys, and the individual child. Issues involving the home as a microsystem include home safety and parent-child interaction. The child care environment or the neighborhood play group are other microsystems that include the child at diflemnt times. Intersections between microsystems were designated as the mesosystem. Connections between home and school or school and child care are important to the developing child. For example, a parent who has difficulties communicating with a caregiver may miss important information that would help them as parents in their child’s home environment. The exosystem includes areas affecting the child indirectly. For example, parental work demands may affect their volunteer work at their children’s preschool, thus affecting the child. The microsystem, or the environment in contact with the child, is affected by the mesosystem, which in turn is affected by the exosystem. The macrosystem includes broader attitudes and ideologies, i.e., culture, socioeconomic status, values, and government. In the previous example, the parent may be reluctant to quit a demanding job because of a desire to maintain the family’s socioeconomic status. The macrosystem may influence this parent’s beliefs to value spending time developing their career over spending time volunteering in their child’s preschool. Bronfenbrenner’s (1992, p. 197) later “person-process-context model” emphasized the dynamic nature of development through interaction between people and their environments. It is recognized that the environment affects children; however, children make changes in their environment also. A more sociable child is more likely to 16 attract attention from teachers and peers. Researchers were encouraged to include both the environment and the individual in their work, rather than focusing solely on the influence of settings or the attributes of pe0ple. Bronfenbrenner’s (2000, p. 129) current model, termed the “bioecological model” accounts for heritable traits and the effect of the environment on the individual over time. The environment may include objects, symbols, and people; it may be in proximal contact with the individual as in the microsystem of the ecological model or it may be more remote, such as the neighborhood or the government. The individual develops in the context of the environment through proximal processes that vary as a function of the person, the nature of developmental outcomes, and the continuities and discontinuities that occur over time, i.e., process-person-context-time. Furthermore, developmental processes are enhanced when they involve another individual who has developed a close relationship with the child. Thus, this dissertation will focus more on parent-child relationships and the home environment than child care provider-child relationships and the child care environment. Family resources, child temperament, and other characteristics of the person and environment will be included. Although Bronfenbrenner’s (1979) emphasis on the study of development in context was followed by his colleagues, he found that his overemphasis on the environment as a context for development created a new problem, “a surfeit of studies on context without development.” (Bronfenbrenner & Morris, 1998, p. 994). Thus, this dissertation examines not only the context and the process of parenting but the development of children. In particular, a focus on social-emotional development includes attachment security and child self-control as outcome measures. 17 Ecological theory provides a framework for this dissertation; attachment theory and social cognitive theory provide a basis for investigation. Attachment theory attempts to explain the relation between the child’s environment, parental behavior, and child behavior (Cassidy & Shaver, 1999); social cognitive theory suggests that parental efficacy mediates between the environment and parental behavior or between the environment and attachment (Bandura, 1995; Donovan & Leavitt, 1989; Gondoli & Silverberg, 1997; Luster & Rhoades, 1989; Teti & Gelfand, 1991). The following section discusses social cognitive theory and attachment theory as a basis for this research. Social Cognitive Theory Rotter, an early leader in the study of efficacy, developed the idea of a locus of control (1990). An internal locus of control was defined as the perception that a person’s behavior affected his environment. An external locus of control was defined as the perception that luck, chance, or external forces beyond personal control afl‘ected a person’s environment. Instead of using the language of locus of control, Bandura (1982) explained personal control, or the perception of a connection between efforts and outcomes, as self- efficacy. A major tenet of Bandura’s social cognitive theory is that learning occurs by 91Mngggdeled_behayi9r; gertainly observation (of other individuals: effectiveness ~1;e_l_2_1_te_ks_t(_iself-motivationaryiiieficflgacy. However, personal experiences of success or failure are more relevant to motivation. According to Bandura, individuals’ beliefs about the connection between their efforts and the desired outcomes affect their motivation to act. Theoretically, those who have higher levels of self-efficacy are more likely to be 18 motivated to succeed at a given task. for example, parents who believe that they can affect their children’s actions by their efforts (e.g., help them learn to talk by talking to them) are more likely to spend time and effort in parenting. A second dimension of self- efficacy, also discussed by Bandura (1982, 1989), involves perceived competence. For example, parents who felt that they were skilled and capable in handling their children are more apt to spend time engaged with their children because of the personal satisfaction they receive. In his later work, Bandura cited attribution theory to differentiate the relation between perceived control and perceived competence: “Self-efficacy beliefs [perceived competence] influence causal attributions [perceived control]. People who regard themselves as highly efiicacious attribute their failures to insufficient effort, those who regard themselves as inefficacious attribute their failures to low ability” (Bandura, 1994). Although Bandura has redefined the relation between perceived competence and perceived control, research continues to use varied conceptualizations of efficacy and its dimensions. In addition to problematic conceptualizations within the field, a debate about the boundaries of self-efficacy exists (Coleman & Karraker, 1997). Is self-efficacy a personality trait, or is it a state that varies over time? Self-efficacy theory developed by Bandura describes efficacy as a domain-specific construct; Harter (1978) prefers a global orientation. According to Bandura (1989), efficacy in one area does not apply to efficacy in another; however, a multi-faoeted approach to efficacy measurement may improve measurement capabilities. In a previous study, a latent construct of multiple measures of child efficacy was a significant predictor in structural equation modeling, although the 19 measures of efficacy were not intereorrelated (Bandura, Barbaranelli, Caprara, & Pastorelli, 1996). Individuals may feel competent in one area but may feel deficient in another; similarly they may feel that they have control in some situations but not in others. Bandura has suggested that measures be constructed from task-specific items, such as “I feel capable of soothing my child when he/she is upset,” rather than global items, such as “I feel like I am a good mother.” Parental Eflicacy Self-efficacy that encompasses parenting is defined here as parental efficacy. Parental eflicacy, or the belief in one’s ability to parent effectively, is composed of various domains/I.e828 8222 82828.2 8.2 8282222222280 .882 - .52. em 2. .2.. eo2. .2. eoN. .2.» 2. coo. 2.2 2.- .n2.- .oo. ooo. 2922-2238223 .m2 2. - oo. sm2.- *oN. eon. acm- sa2. .2. 2.02.- ..2.- swo. *2. 282028282032 .2 2o. 22o. - mot 2.02. 8mm. awn. 828m. 8N2. an 2.- .802 .- mo. oo- 23 2.822220 .2 2 Not 5.. No. - 2&2.- ..oor eon: somr 2.3.- ...nm. .58. .236 .nm.- 82:28 282.332 .3 8.2 2. .82. no. no.- - an2. «nN. 8on- 822. 2.3.- 8.02.- ..o2. 8N2. 802282338220 V228-2 .o no. co. vo. not goo. - 2.228. .222. no. m2.- 2; 2o. no. 3222229382 32828082 .w 8N2. .822. 2.92. no.- *2. com. - enm. *NN. anmr awn: no. 822. 2208:2202-222m .n .2. .82. mo. Nor .oo. *oo. «2. - 2.9.. .n2.- soNr No. .2. b2>222mcom .o no. mo. oo. mot emo. no. .8222. 8mm. - *m2.- 2&2.- vo. no. 85228222822m .m 3. cor 2o. .3. wot oo.- «no: 5. mo. - ion. {emf *nm.- .02: m2 ammo .2. mo: 2.2.. Sr now. wot no.- no.- 2o.- vor 8mm. - 2.228.- 2.3...- .oE v.2 ammo .m 28o. mo. 2o.- .52.- mo. vo. mo: 3. mo: _..n2.- 8.98.- - 2.3.. 0082208288 2898208082 .N No. .52. mo. 2&2.- mor No. goo. mo. not 2.2 2.- *mNr enm. - 295:8 28282092082 .2 m2 .2 22 3 o m n o m 2. m N 2 40243.ch amen-IN» 3228.8 25 8.832.292 w 828228-2- 72 Analyses of variance. Analyses of variance tested relations between demographic groups and continuous variables. These tests were used as preliminary contrasts of processes. Multiple comparison procedures, such as MANOVAs, were not used in these preliminary analyses. Multiple comparison procedures followed by post-hoe tests delete participant information that is incomplete. These procedures may result in false negatives; therefore, some statisticians recommend such methods only for determining sets of variables, i.e., with multiple regression (Ludbrook, 1998). Although Type I errors may result fi'om multiple comparisons, my goal is not solely to determine significance but rather to perform preliminary tests of effects. Comparisons of study variables between Afiican Americans and Caucasians are displayed in Table 9. In order to ensure that differences in process were not due to unequal variances within groups, Levene’s test of equality of variance was included in this table. Comparisons by economic groups are displayed in Table 10. (See Appendix B for comparisons by race and income.) Tests of gender differences are included in Table 11. Parent characteristics, including parental perceived control, perceived competence, maternal depression, ratings of mother-child interaction, parenting as measured by subscales of the HOME, and marital quality were tested for significant differences between groups. Child-related variables, including child care quality, 36- month attachment, and self-control, were also tested for differences. 73 .28. V Q8: on.2 9.6 So 5828-32280 2 2. own 2.... coo- No.2 2 2.22 3 2 2282-2258 22822.2( H822822282288 88. SN 8... 228228280 ow. we. 2.... ood- oo. and 2.82 22.82.2252. 22822:. 2282822283 no.8. mod oeo 5828322280 mo. 2o.m .:.. o8; Nmn 8.3 3.2 22822252. 52...: 2.8222 82 Q-mmu nno n~.o 2vo2 2828322280 no. mn.m .:.. 228 eon 822.2 mn2 502.208... 828222.82 2.8282 0.2 Q-mmo mn- 2o.v ~32 582882280 2n. v2. 5. 2m. mm. Nod mn2 2282-22528 22822.32. 238282288 282823-2082 we. ond ~32 282832280 .:.... mm.» mo. oo. mm. on...“ ~n2 22822252. 582.52. 22825... 289828-2882 2o.~ nm.m 2.82 52882280 .2.: 8.28 :3 $.82- Nm.2 «2.2 mn2 2232-2252 22828-222. 2088222 .8248- 38138 28 2228-2 828 8 2. 8. 28.88I. > 8 822288 74 48. V Q2... mu. 3. new 523250 9.. 2.. .:.. mod- 3. 5..- a2 2 2232.85 §E< ”20.52.00.223 cm. 2m. 93 52.8280 mm. on. 2:. mod. 2. cm. 2: 592.2223. 5352‘ ”2220822852. «3. Kama moo 5228250 mm. 3. .:.. $6. 3.2V 3.2 mm 2232.232 §E< ”E23: 28 22.6 m». 2 N. N mg 52.0.3280 9: NYMM .22.. no.0 2.2 end mv— anew—oi §E< H822.2223 282.82 8.2 o2.m 223 5298280 3.... £242 .:.... N 2 .m- am; NW? 9..— gov—05 §E< 53283: .3 V203 an. 20d 03 528030 .:.... owdv I... cod- 2m._ mad m: 2282.85 5054‘ “$2225.23 3222832 2 .2 222 .5 03 2823250 .:.... Nvdm— .2:... 3».— 2- ocN mN.m m: 502.205 §E< ”uaogotcm ma 0%3 42m 2.29.» a H a 2 23> 8822 um E22923 9.282 a 2.3 75 Table 10 Stu_dz variables by economic groyg Variable n M SD t-value Sig. Perceived control: Low-income 357 3.68 .55 -4.40 *" Middle income 600 3.83 .47 Perceived competence: Low-income 357 3.95 .52 -1 .96 .05 Middle income 600 4.02 .53 CES-D 1-6 mo.: Low-income 357 13.74 7.31 13.60 * " Middle income 600 7.97 5.70 CBS-D 15 mo.: Low-income 292 12.20 7.76 10.22 *" Middle income 573 6.91 5.96 Stimulation: Low-income 305 2.33 .62 -9.74 *** Middle income 583 2.77 .64 Sensitivity: Low-income 305 8.25 1.75 -12.90 "" Middle income 583 9.80 1.59 Enrichment: Low-income 288 5.82 1.97 -13.1 1 *" Middle income 573 7.42 .87 Positive parenting: Low-income 288 5.10 1.21 -8.61 “W Middle income 573 5.75 .57 Lack of negativity: Low-income 288 4.56 1.33 -8.81 "* Middle income 573 5.32 .91 Marital conflict: Low-income 288 2.70 1.13 7.71 *" Middle income 595 2. 13 .75 "* p < .001. 76 Table 10 (cont’d) Variable n M SD t-me Sig. Child care quality: Low-income 140 1 8.91 5.27 -8.92 "' “ "' Middle income 399 23.35 4.44 Attachment: Low-income 277 .22 .20 -6.77 ”" Middle income 555 .32 .20 Self-control: Low-income 251 -.25 .71 -6.93 **‘" Middle income 506 .13 .71 "‘ p < .001. 77 Table 11 Stu variables b ender Variable n M SD t—value Sig. Perceived control: Male 704 3.79 .49 1 .23 .22 Female 659 3.76 .50 Perceived competence: Male 704 3.99 .55 -1.00 .32 Female 659 4.02 .52 CBS-D 1-6 mo.: Male 705 10.02 6.83 .22 .83 Female 658 9.94 6.78 CES-D 15 mo.: Male 637 8.52 7.10 -.72 .47 Female 604 8.81 7.06 Stimulation: Male 658 9.15 1.81 -1.25 .21 Female 614 9.28 1.75 Sensitivity: Male 658 2.55 .65 -2.37 .02 Female 614 2.64 .66 Enrichment: Male 633 6.87 1.41 -.96 .34 Female 601 6.95 1 .49 Positive parenting: Male 633 5.47 .98 -2.66 .01 Female 601 5.60 .77 Lack of negativity: Male 633 4.91 1.17 -4.45 ”“ Female 601 5.19 1.03 "’p < .001. 78 Table 11 (cont’d) Variable n M SD t-value Sig. Marital conflict: Male 658 2.29 .89 -1.34 .18 Female 613 2.36 .97 Child care quality: Male 404 22.12 4.98 -.04 .97 Female 369 22.13 4.62 Attachment: Male 614 .27 .21 -4.50 ”" Female 583 .32 .20 Self-control: Male 564 -.06 .79 -2.97 ”* Female 529 .07 .67 "‘ p < .001. 79 Results indicated that African American mothers experienced more depression and marital conflict. African American families displayed less sensitive mother-child interaction and had lower HOME scores. There were no differences between racial groups on the parental efficacy measures of perceived control and perceived competence. However, low-income mothers had lower scores on both dimensions of parental efficacy than middle-to-high income mothers. Low-income mothers were also significantly more depressed, less sensitive, and stimulating with their children. There were significant differences in the expected direction between economic groups on all other study variables (HOME scores, marital conflict, child care quality, 24-month attachment security, and self-control). Tests of gender differences indicated that boys received more positive parenting and negative parenting (i.e., more positive attention and negative attention) than girls. Mothers behaved more sensitively toward girls than boys during videotaped mother-child interaction. Girls were more securely attached than boys and exhibited more self-control. Attachment Security. One-way analyses of variance with quality of attachment (avoidant, secure, resistant) as the factor were conducted for continuous variables. Only variables measured in infancy (either concurrent with or previous to strange situation measurement) were included in these analyses of variance. These include parental perceived control, perceived competence, global ratings of sensitivity and stimulation, depression, marital quality, and child care quality. Results are presented in Table 12. T- tests were completed for attachment as a 2-way factor (security/insecurity). For these comparisons, disorganized attachment was included in the insecure group. Relations between study variables and attachment security follow in Table 13. 80 Table 12 Means ofstudy variables by attachment classification with one-way analyses of variance Variable A B C F Contrasts Income 2.48 3.12 3.30 9.32* A < B, A < C (195) (817) (175) Perceived control 3.97 4.01 3.94 .10 (195) (819) (176) Perceived competence 3.97 4.01 3.95 1.33 (195) (819) (176) CESD 1-6 mo. 10.34 9.67 10.47 1.53 (195) (819) (176) Stimulation 2.50 2.62 2.66 3.21 * (189) (808) (173) Sensitivity 8.90 9.26 9.50 5.52“ A < B, A < C (189) (808) (173) Enrichment 6.65 6.95 7.04 4.13“ A < B, A < C (193) (813) (174) Positive parenting 5.34 5.59 5.51 6.58" A < B (193) (813) (174) Lack of negativity 4.80 5.11 5.13 6.74" A < B, A < C (193) (813) (174) Marital conflict 2.43 2.29 2.30 1.71 (174) (780) (168) Child care 20.68 22.63 21.90 9.04"' A < B (130) 4483) (102) Notes: Ns are in parentheses. " p < .05. A = avoidant, B = secure, C = resistant. 81 Table 13 Stm variables by attachment securig Variable n M SD t-vw Sig Perceived control: Secure 742 3.78 .50 -.05 .96 Insecure 448 3.78 .47 Perceived competence: Secure 742 3.78 .52 1 .76 .08 Insecure 448 3.96 .55 CBS-D 1-6 mo.: Secure 742 9.63 6.59 -1.73 .08 Insecure 448 10.34 6.97 CES-D 15 mo.: Secure 738 8.71 7.07 .1 1 .91 Insecure 448 8.66 7.08 Stimulation: Secure 732 9.30 1.74 1.55 .12 Insecure 438 9.13 1.81 Sensitivity: Secure 732 2.62 .64 1.21 .23 Insecure 438 2.58 .67 Enrichment: Secure 736 6.99 1 .39 2.26 .02 Insecure 444 6.79 1.53 Positive parenting: Secure 736 5.60 .78 3.20 ‘“ Insecure 444 5.42 1.02 Lack of negativity: Secure 736 5.13 1.04 2.51 .01 Insecure 444 4.95 1.19 m p < .001. 82 Table 13 (cont’d) Variable n M SD t—value Sig. Marital conflict: Secure 707 2.26 .87 -2.44 .02 Insecure 415 2.40 .99 Childcare: Secure 437 22.79 4.66 4.48 *** Insecure 278 21.19 4.70 Attachment: Secure 71 1 .30 .20 2.13 .03 Insecure 431 .28 .21 Self-control: Secure 657 .03 .72 1.12 .26 Insecure 384 -.03 .76 m p < .001. 83 Analyses with study variables and attachment by classification and by a dichotomous test of security/insecurity yielded interesting results. First, one-way analyses of variance indicated that income, all parenting variables, and child care quality were related to attachment classification. Insecure-avoidant children came fiom families with lower income, received less sensitive and positive parenting but more negative parenting, had less enriched home environments, and received lower quality child care than securely attached children. Children classified as insecure-resistant received more sensitive parenting, less negative parenting, and had more enriched home environments than insecure-avoidant children, but there were no differences in mother-child interaction or other study variables between insecure-resistant children and secure children. The second set of analyses (secure/insecure attachment) indicated that home enrichment, positive parenting, and quality child care were associated with secure attachment. Negative parenting and marital conflict were associated with insecure attachment. Children who were securely attached at 15 months had significantly higher scores on the Attachment Q—Set at 24 months. Sensitivity. Preliminary tests of mother-child interaction ratings indicated some interesting differences between demographic groups. To review the statistical differences, African American mothers were 3 times more likely to be rated as detached or as displaying negative parenting than Caucasians. Low-income mothers were 3 times more likely to be classified as detached and 4 times more likely to be rated as displaying negative parenting than middle-to-high income mothers. Neither rating was significantly related to a dichotomized measure of attachment security measured at 15 months; however, both ratings were significantly related to attachment security at 24 months. 84 Sensitivity and stimulation were related to attachment classification within the Caucasian group but not within the African American group. Afiican Americans received significantly lower ratings than Caucasians on stimulation and on the composite measure of sensitivity (See Table 9). The HOME inventory subscales also measured parent-child interaction. It is possible that the Positive Parenting subscale provides a more effective alternative to the global sensitivity rating. The two measures have only a small association (r = .21). The Positive Parenting subscale was significantly associated with the Attachment Q-Set in the expected direction for both African Americans and Caucasians. The two measures approach the concept of sensitivity from different perspectives (NICHD, 1997); therefore, global sensitivity and the Positive Parenting subscale are both used in subsequent analyses. Temperament. Maternal assessments of temperament using the Early Infancy Temperament Questionnaire were initially included in the correlation matrix; however, these tests indicated that the measures were problematic. Correlations with self-control ranged from .03 to .06. The correlation between global and mean assessments was -.30 (p < .01). Documentation noted that the Early Infancy Temperament Questionnaire had not been previously validated (NICHD, 1999a). Although initial plans called for use of the Infant Temperament Questionnaire, a more established measure, at the 6-month assessment, maternal assessments of child temperament were discontinued after the 1- month data collection. The Infant Temperament Questionnaire was not used at the 1- month timepoint because it was considered to be developmentally inappropriate for 1- month-old infants. Conceptually, a measure of temperament would have been useful in 85 evaluating these data; however, the existing measure proved to be inadequate and was not used in this dissertation. Tests of Hypotheses Hypotheses are grouped in four major areas. First, tests of hypotheses related to perceived control are displayed in Table 14. Results are then discussed. Second, tests of hypotheses related to perceived competence as presented in Table 15 are discussed. Third, results of the logistic regressions are reviewed (Hypothesis 2 — 3). Fourth, results of the structural equation models are reviewed. Table 14 Hypotheses aid tests related to perceiveaL control Hyp_othesis Empirical Test 1a: Parents with higher levels of perceived Significant correlations between control are more likely to exhibit sensitive Parental Locus of Control and global behavior. ratings of mother-child interaction 1b: Parents with higher levels of perceived Analysis of variance on Parental control are more likely to have infants rated Locus of Control with attachment as securely attached. security/insecurity as the factor 1c: The relation between perceived control and Contrast analysis comparing effects secure attachment will be stronger among between low-income families and low-income families than middle-to-high middle-to-high income families; 2- income families. score significance test. 1d: The relation between perceived control and Contrast analysis comparing effects attachment security will be stronger among between African American and African American families than Caucasian Caucasian families; z-score families. significance test. 86 Perceived control. For the total sample, maternal sensitivity at six months was significantly correlated to perceived control at one month (p = .001, n = 1272). However, this relation bordered on a small effect size (r = .09). Cohen’s (1988) convention for correlational effect sizes indicates the range for small effect sizes as .10 to .30. Although it is possible to reject the null hypothesis for Hypothesis 1a as stated, the practical significance may be overestimated because of the large sample size. Tests using a large sample size may infer a significant effect that is only trivial. Among African Americans, maternal perceived control showed a more substantial relation to maternal sensitivity (r = .19, p = .016, n = 154). The relation between perceived control and sensitivity was not significant among Caucasians. Among low-income families, maternal perceived control was also related to sensitivity (r = .17, p = .003, n = 305). However, no significant association was found between perceived control and sensitivity among middle-to-high income families. Hypothesis 1b was not confirmed by significance testing. Mothers with securely attached children did not have significantly higher perceived control scores than mothers with insecurely attached children. Insecure attachment was defined as avoidant, resistant, or disorganized attachment. Although perceived control was not related to strange situation classification at 15 months, perceived control was related to attachment security at 24 months as measured by the Attachment Q-Set (r = .11,p < .001, n = 1197). Hypothesis 1c tests the relation between perceived control and attachment for African Americans and Caucasians. No significant relation existed when attachment was measured at 15 months for either group. However, perceived control predicted attachment security at 24 months among African Americans (r = .26, p = .002, n = 141). 87 Furthermore, there was no significant relation between perceived control and attachment security among Caucasians at 24 months (r = .06, p = .06, n = 930). To test for significant differences between the two correlations, the coefficients were transformed with the Fisher-Z transform (Papoulis, 1990). The difference of the two z-scores was adjusted for sample size by dividing by the square root of the sum of inverse sample sizes minus 3 [1/(N1-3) + 1/(N2-3)]. The resulting z-value was significant (p < .02). This test provided partial support for concluding that the relation between perceived control and attachment is stronger among African Americans than among Caucasians. Hypothesis 1d tests the relation between perceived control and attachment by income groups. No significant relation existed when attachment was measured at 15 months for either group. In low-income families, perceived control tended to predict attachment security at 24 months (r = .1 1, p < .08, n = 277). In middle-to-high income families, the correlation effect size was similar (r = . 10, p < .02, n = 555). The difference in significance level between the two groups was related to the sample size. The Fisher-Z contrast was not significant (p < .98). Thus, it is not possible to reject the null hypothesis; findings indicate no real difference in the effect of perceived control on attachment between economic groups. Tests of perceived competence follow in Table 15. 88 Table 15 Hypotheses and tests 01 aerceived competence anothesia 2: Perceived competence and parenting style will be related to attachment classification. 2a: Children displaying an insecure-avoidant attachment are likely to have mothers with high levels of perceived competence. 2b: Children displaying an insecure-resistant attachment are likely to have mothers with low levels of perceived competence. 2c: Children displaying a secure attachment are likely to have mothers with an average level of perceived competence, greater than that of mothers in insecure-resistant dyads and less than that of mothers in insecure-avoidant dyads. 2d: Children displaying insecure attachment are likely to have mothers who are less sensitive. 2e: Relations between parental efficacy, parenting style, and attachment will vary between ethnic and income groups. Empirical Test Tests 2 (a - f) and Model )(2 indicates fit of a logistic regression Significance of contrasts in analysis of variance (preliminary test) Significance of contrasts in analysis of variance (prelimimry test) Significance of contrasts in analysis of variance (preliminary test) Significance for ratings of parenting behavior, analysis of variance (preliminary test) Within-group tests of Hypotheses 2a through 2d for African Americans, Caucasians, and economic groups Perceived competence. Preliminary tests of perceived competence, corresponding to Hypotheses 2a — 2d, are discussed in this section. Results of the logistic regression of perceived competence and parenting on attachment security (Hypothesis 2) follow. Preliminary AN OVAs indicated that perceived competence did not predict strange situation classification for the total sample. For African Americans, however, perceived competence was a significant predictor of attachment classification, F (2, 134) 89 = 3.34, p < .04. Greater variance within the African American sample may have explained these results; however, a test for equality of variance with Caucasians was not significant, F(2, 1212) = .14,p < .71. Post-hoe tests indicated that Afi'ican American children with insecure-avoidant attachments did not have mothers with the highest levels of competence as suggested in Hypothesis 2a. However, insecure-avoidant children did have mothers with high scores on this measure (M = 4.03) that compared favorably with scores of mothers with secure children (M = 4.07). Children with insecure-resistant attachments did have mothers with the lowest mean scores on this measure (M = 3.74), as suggested in Hypothesis 2b. Differences on this measure between insecure-resistant and secure groups reached significance, indicating that children displaying a secure attachment were more likely to have mothers with a level of perceived competence greater than that of mothers in insecure-resistant dyads. This provided partial support for Hypothesis 2c. Within-group tests of Caucasian, low-income, and middle-to—high income families produced no significant relations between perceived competence and strange situation classification. Thus, Hypotheses 2(a — c) were supported only among the African American sample. The Positive Parenting subscale of the HOME as measured at 15 months was also used as a measure of parenting style in the following analyses. For the total sample, the positive parenting scale was a significant predictor of attachment classification, F (2, 1177) = 6.58, p < .001. Post-hoe tests indicated that mothers with insecure-avoidant children displayed significantly less positive parenting than mothers with securely 90 attached children. In general, preliminary tests indicated that positive parenting was related to strange situation classification at 15 months (Hypothesis 2d). Table 16 below presents results for the measures of parenting style, including global sensitivity and positive parenting. Sensitivity was a significant predictor of attachment classification for the total sample (n = 1169). Post-hoe tests indicated that mothers with insecure-avoidant children displayed significantly less sensitivity than mothers with securely attached children. Means of positive parenting scores and tests of significance by demographic groups are also displayed in Table 16. Both sensitivity and positive parenting were associated with attachment security (Hypothesis 2d). There were, however, differences by demographic groups (Hypothesis 2e). For example, positive parenting was not a significant predictor of strange situation classification among low-income families. Sensitivity did not predict strange situation classification among African American or low-income groups. Furthermore, mothers with insecure-resistant children were rated as more sensitive than mothers with secure children among middle- to-high income families. 91 Table 16 Parenting measures and strange sthuation classification with demogaahic gzouas Meaas A B C F Contrasts Sensitivity: African Americans 8.03 8.22 7.56 1.01 (30) (86) (18) Caucasians 9.16 9.46 9.83 5.49“ A < C (138) (634) (138) Low-income 8.07 8.21 8.11 .79 (60) (155) (35) Mid/high income 9.41 9.76 10.25 5.73“ A < C, B < C (59) (358) (84) Positive parenting: African Americans 4.58 5.24 4.37 5.64" A < B, B > C (31) (85) (19) Caucasians 5.44 5.64 5.67 4.43" A < B, A < C (140) (638) (138) Low-income 4.95 5.17 5.18 .79 (65) (169) (38) Mid/high income 5.62 5.77 5.80 2.401 (74) (389) (91) Note: A = avoidant, B = secure, C = resistant. ns for groups are in parentheses. "p < .01, ‘p< .05, *p < .10 Logistic regressions. Logistic regressions were used to test hypotheses for attachment security in infancy (Hypotheses 2 - 3). First, a multinomial regression including parental efficacy as perceived competence, parenting style, and income as a control variable tested Hypothesis 2. In multinomial logistic regression, the dependent 92 variable is nominal but not restricted to two categories (Hosmer & Lemeshow, 1989). In addition to model fit, significance of individual variables tested Hypothesis 2a — 2e. Second, logistic regressions including child care interactions and control variables examine Hypothesis 3. These regressions examine differences in the environments of children with secure and insecure attachments. Based on preliminary tests, income was included as a control variable. Gender differences were tested as a control because of previous findings related to attachment security (NICHD, 2001). However, in these analyses, no effects were found. A measure of income-to-needs ratio averaging three timepoints (1 month, 6 months, 15 months) was constructed to exclude inappropriate information (24 months, 36 months) from these analyses of 15-month attachment security. The fit of the multinomial logistic regression model with perceived competence, positive parenting, and income was significant for the full sample, 12 (6) = 28.52, df= 6, p = .000, n = 1147). However, the effect size index of Nagelkerke, a pseudo r-square measure, was trivial (.03). In simpler terms, this model explained only 3% of the variance in attachment quality. Table 17 displays significant betas for this regression. 93 Table 17 Mdtinomial logistic regression model of attachment classification for the total samale Avoidant Resistant Income -.13 .06 (.17) (.24) Perceived Competence -. 10 -.27T (.15) (.53) Positive Parenting -.21 * -.14 (.08) (.10) Intercept .46 .12* (.74) (.79) n 193 173 Notes: N = 1177. Reference category is secure attachment. Standard errors in parentheses. * p < .05, Tp < .10 A test of this regression among Caucasians was similar to the test of the total sample, with significant model fit but insubstantial effect sizes. The only significant beta was that of income. The fit of the same model using data from Afiican Americans was considerably improved. The likelihood ratio test indicated model fit, 12 (6, N = 135) = 15.94, df= 6, p = .005). The Nagelkerke index reached .14, indicating that the model explained 14% of the variance in attachment classification. Table 18 below displays significant betas for this regression. 94 Table 18 Multinomial logi_stic regression model for African American families Avoidant Resistant Income .04 -.13 (.17) (.24) Perceived Competence -.06 -1.1 1* (.42) (.53) Positive Parenting -.42* -.46* (.17) (.20) Intercept 1 .27 5 .24" (1.86) (2.19) n 31 19 Notes: n = 135. Reference category is secure attachment. Standard errors in parentheses. *p < .05, Tp < .10 Within the African American sample, income was not a predictor of attachment classification. Mothers with lower perceived competence were more likely to have children with insecure-resistant classifications but not insecure-avoidant classification. Mothers whose children had insecure-avoidant classification did not have lower scores than mothers whose children were rated as secure. Positive parenting predicted secure classification. Models divided by income groups did not reach a significant fit. In sum, findings provide partial support for Hypothesis 2, which states, in simple terms, that mothers who are insensitive and have children with insecure-avoidant classification (Type A) may overestimate their competence as parents. This effect is most noticeable among African 95 Americans, who had greater variance in parenting style than Caucasians (Levine’s test for equality of variance, F = 48.89, p < .001). The following logistic regressions, testing hypotheses related to child care quality, ( Table 19) examines only the total sample because of the reduced number of participants related to child care participation. Specific interactions that are mentioned in Hypothesis 3 include depression and parenting style. Table 19 Hmtheses an_d tests of moderators of child care quality Hypothgsi_s Empirical Test 3: For children in child care, child care quality With interaction terms, Model X may moderate the effect of other variables indicates fit of the logistic on attachment. regression 3a: The quality of child care may buffer the Wald 12 statistic for interaction term likelihood of insecure attachment for of child care quality (APECP or depressed mothers. APFDC) x depression (CBS-D) 3b: The quality of child care may buffer the Wald 1.2 statistic for interaction term likelihood of insecure attachment for mothers of child care quality (APECP or with insensitive parenting. APF DC) x average global ratings Two logistic regressions with a dichotomous insecure/secure dependent variable were performed on the total sample. The first regression tested child care as a moderator of the relation between depression and attachment security. In this equation, only child care quality was a significant predictor (.12 = 19.02, p < .001). Thus, Hypothesis 3a is not confirmed. The second regression tested the interaction of positive parenting x child care as a moderator of the relation between child attachment security. In this model, the 96 interaction term was significant (.12 = 17.62, p < .001). The model fit was also adequate (f = 18.32, p < .001, n = 705). Thus, children who experience positive home environments and quality child care are more likely to be securely attached than those who rate highly on only quality child care or the positive home environment measure. These results provide support for Hypothesis 3b. The following section describes . structural equation models testing relations with attachment security at 24 months and child self-control at 36 months. Structural equation modeling. Structural equation models specify a causal order among a set of variables (Klein, 1998) and then assess the extent to which the predicted interrelationships differ from the interrelationships actually observed in the data. However, structural equation modeling does not imply causality. Causality is more probable if certain conditions are met, such as temporal ordering of path variables and the elimination of other possible causes through experimental controls. If a model adequately describes actual relations between variables, then the model is said to fit the data, as in Hypothesis 4, “The model describing the development of mother-child attachment and child self-control as displayed in Figure 2 will fit the data.” A chi-square statistic is used to test whether the differences between the obtained and the predicted data are greater than zero. A significant chi-square indicates an implausible model because the differences are too large. To reduce the tendency of chi-square to be inflated by sample size, the chi-square is divided by the degrees of freedom. If the quotient meets the commonly-accepted criteria of being less than 3.0, the data will be said to fit the model (Kline, 1998). 97 A number of other fit indices are available to evaluate the degree of congruence between a model and data. The goodness-of-fit index (GFI) considers the proportion of variance in the data accounted for by a given model (Ullman, 1996). It can range from 0.0 (no fit at all) to 1.0 (a perfect fit). A GFI of 0.9 is generally accepted as indicating a good fit (Bentler & Bonett, 1980). The Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA) reflects the differences between the observed and the model-implied covariance. RMSEA of 0.0 would indicate a perfect fit of the model to the data. In practice, 0.05 or less is considered to indicate a close fit (Browne & Cudeck, 1993). Structural equation modeling requires a set of complete data. Missing data are likely to result in less accurate computations than when data are replaced by estimation of maximum likelihood (Little & Rubin, 1989). Missing data also make inferences to the general population less meaningful. Thus, we created substitutions for missing values with a maximum likelihood method based on the Estimation Maximization (EM) algorithm (Dempster, Laird, & Rubin, 1977). Maximum likelihood methods estimate the means and covariance matrix of study variables, rather than simply estimating single missing values (Little & Rubin, 1987). Only cases that had a child outcome variable, either self-control or attachment security, were included in the missing data analyses. Although the total sample size is more than adequate, division of the sample into groups makes certain analyses difficult. The number of African American families (n = 176) that had one or more child variables (n = 144) is too small for an accurate analysis using structural equation modeling. Thus, this method was not used for comparisons of processes across racial groups. 98 Structural equation models tested a modified model similar to Figure 2. Preliminary tests eliminated temperament as a variable; thus temperament was not included in the final model. Marital quality was included in separate structural equation models including only participants who had data on the marital conflict measure. Paths linking variables were used as tests for Hypotheses 4a — 4e, displayed in Table 20. 99 Table 20 Hmtheses and corresaandingsignificance tests of variable-level path; Hypothesis Test of path significance 4a-1: Mothers who have higher perceived Parental Locus of Conn-019 control are more likely to display positive Attachment Q-Set parenting practices. 4a-2: Mothers who have higher perceived control are less likely to be depressed. 4b: Mothers who are less depressed are more likely to display positive parenting practices. 40: Families who experience less marital conflict are more likely to have positive parenting practices. 4d-1: Parents who display positive parenting practices (e. g., sensitivity) are more likely to have children with secure attachments. 4d-2: Parents who display positive parenting practices are likely to have children with greater self-control. 4e: Relations between parenting practices, attachment, and self-control may vary between ethnic and economic groups. Parental Locus of Control9 CBS-D CES-D 9 HOME Love and Relationships 9 HOME HOME 9 Attachment Q-Set HOME 9 Self Control Procedure Comparisons of structural equation models 100 Eight models describing the linkages among characteristics of maternal perceived control and maternal depression, children’s home environment, and child outcomes are presented. The first model used data from the total sample; the second model, which included a variable of marital conflict, excluded single parent families. The next two models compared differences in process by child gender. The last four models described low-income and middle-to-high income samples, with and without the marital conflict variable. The first model tested the predictors of child attachment and self-control with marital conflict omitted. Maternal perceived control and maternal depression were co- varying exogenous variables. Positive mother-child interaction, negative mother-child interaction, enrichment, and attachment were the four endogenous variables. Paths between positive and negative mother-child interaction indicated that these variables had a bi-directional effect, resulting in a nonrecursive model. This nonrecursive subset had a stability index of .36. Indices below 1 are considered to be stable. Figure 4 displays the model with path coefficients. All paths were significant excepting the path between perceived control and positive mother-child interaction. The path between positive mother-child interaction and self-control indicated a trend only (p < .10). Goodness-of-fit measures indicated that the data fit the model, confirming Hypothesis 4. The [I test was not significant, indicating a good fit, 12 (3) = 3.03, p = .39. Two additional indices also indicated that the interpretation of the estimated paths is acceptable (RMSEA = .003, GFI = .999). 101 28803228 223 «5822083 .20 $290an .23 20258 cows—225 gogm .2» 953k ;/., \\ ./ .228 . 828.8 l C..- 102 Two additional models examined gender differences. The structure of these models was identical to the first model. Goodness of fit was acceptable for boys, [2 (3) = 5.55, p = .14, RMSEA = .037, GFI = .998, n = 631; similar fit was achieved for girls, )8 (3) = 5.66, p = .13, RMSEA = .039, GFI = .997, n = 598. Path coefficients by gender are presented in Table 21. An enriched home environment and attachment security were related to self-control for boys only. Negative mother-child interactions were related to self-control for girls only. Perceived control predicted home enrichment only for girls. 103 Table 21 Path coeficients and estimates by child gender m Umdized Control 9 Positive Male -.16 Female -.06 Depression 9 Negative Male -.05 Female -.04 Control 9 Enrichment Male .05 Female .25 Positive 9 Enrichment Male .53 Female .56 Lack of Negativity 9 Enrichment Male .23 Female .24 Depression 9Enrichment Male -.04 Female -.02 Control 9 Attachment Male .03 Female .03 Positive 9 Attachment Male .02 Female .03 Enrichment 9 Attachment Male .02 Female .01 Standardized p -.08 .22 -.04 .36 -.31 .02 a26 *** .02 .64 .08 .03 36 ##ll .29 *$* .19 *** .16 ttt -.19 m -.09 .02 .07 .06 .09 .03 .10 .02 .1 1 .01 .16 *** .11 .01 Notes: For boys, n = 631. For girls, n = 598. "*p < .001. 104 Table 21 (cont’d) Paths Unstandardized Standardized D Lack of Negativity 9 Attachment Male .02 .12 *** Female .01 .06 .12 Depression 9 Attachment Male .00 -.07 .09 Female .00 -.08 .04 Lack of Negativity 9 Self-control Male .02 .03 .45 Female .07 .11 .01 Positive 9 Self-control Male .04 .04 .29 Female .04 .05 .28 Enrichment 9 Self-control Male .08 .15 *** Female .08 .19 *** Attachment 9 Self-control Male .52 .14 *** Female .21 .06 .12 Lack of Negativity 9 Positive Male 1.03 1.26 .01 Female .29 .52 .01 Positive 9 Lack of Negativity Male -1.68 -1.38 .08 Female -.51 -.38 .09 m p < .001. 105 A third structural equation tested the model with marital conflict included. For this model test, I created a separate data set limited to children whose mothers were in a married or partnered relationship when the child was 1 month old (n = 1159). The structure of this model was similar to the first model; only marital conflict was added as an endogenous variable. This model (Figure 5) showed additional effects of marital conflict. All path coefficients linked to marital conflict were significant. The direct path between perceived control and home enrichment was no longer significant, but perceived control was indirectly related to home enrichment through marital conflict. All indices indicated a good fit, )8 (5) = 4.62, p = .47, RMSEA = .000, GFI = .999. Maternal depression was related to marital conflict, which in turn, was related to a lack of negativity and home enrichment. Marital conflict had a direct effect on child self-control and indirect effects mediated by parenting variables. Marital conflict also indirectly affected attachment security through its relation to lack of negative parenting. A similar model for boys had an improved fit, f(5) = 3.64, p = .60, n = 595, RMSEA = .000, GFI -—- .998 ; however, the model did not fit for girls, i (5) = 15.84, p = .01, n = 564. Comparative processes by gender are not examined for marital conflict; however, it is interesting that marital conflict appears to have more effect on the development of attachment and self-control among boys. 106 32233.2 .:qubiEm 9.225286 2888228 98 Eon—F233.“ .20 $582253 22223 20258 823525 28322.5 .m. Eami 25562.5 till nae .- 19 a... 2 107 The model was also tested in both economic groups. These models were not tested separately by gender due to the sample size. First, low-income families were included in a model with two-parent and single-parent families (n = 292). Although the model exhibited goodness-of-fit, 12 (3) = 1.25, p = .74, fewer paths were significant than those found in the first model. Figure 6 displays significant paths between variables for the model of the low-income group. Paths between perceived control and study variables were no longer significant. Less depression predicted a lack of negative parenting, which in turn predicted more secure attachments. Positive mother-child interaction was related to attachment both directly and indirectly through home enrichment. Thus, positive parenting predicted home enrichment; both home environment measures were positively related to attachment security. Other paths linking endogenous variables were not significant. The model with marital conflict was also tested (n = 242). The model exhibited goodness-of-fit, f (5) = 1.67, p = .89. However, among low-income families, there were no significant paths between marital conflict and any study variables. This model is displayed in Figure 7. 108 32288.2 6885.302 523 26258 52.5226 gogm 6 Eami KMWV \\ 22.08.202.25 l// \\\ ///,. \100) \\ . . ,/-/ A v \\\\ 8 8 2 - m - 0°.I ///// 2 - . x, no *5 o / / /Mr N 21 22:28.28 . 32% 8 K8 / 2.- 8 cc. 2. T x 2 95:22. W/ \\ 109 2.6222225 883-09% 92225283 .82228am 0885.32 523 26238 concave 28325m N 2:5 110 In the final two models, only families whose income-to-needs ratios equaled or exceeded 3.0 were included (n = 567). The model exhibited goodness-of-fit, f (3) = 2. 12, p = .55, RMSEA = .000, GFI = .999. Perceived control was related to home enrichment, which was related to child self-control. Perceived control was also directly related to attachment security. Negative mother-child interactions predicted less home enrichment and less secure attachments, which in turn predicted less child self-control. Path coefficients are displayed in Figure 8. In the model of middle-to-high income families with single parents excluded (n = 563), the f test confirmed the null hypothesis, indicating no difference between the predicted and observed data, {(5) = 4.27, p = .51. Two additional indices confirmed the goodness of fit (RMSEA = .000, GFI = .998). Figure 9 displays the standardized regression weights for tested paths. Mothers who were depressed were more likely to report marital conflict; mothers with more perceived control were less likely to report marital conflict. Within this group, however, marital conflict was not significantly related to other endogenous variables. 111 8:23. cases 52.282828 523 secs 82.828 2832.5 .m seat 112 «Gaga 32,2098 322:5 528-0236 92225288 .8222Eé 088:2 $363628 523 20258 cone—226 gogw d 959% MU 232.22.: 02v 113 In sum, the structural equation model in Figure 4 supported Hypotheses 4 (a — d). Perceived control was directly related to attachment (Hypothesis 4a) and associated with maternal depression (Hypothesis 4b). In Figure 5, marital conflict was related to negative parenting, and families experiencing marital conflict were less likely to have an enriched home environment (Hypothesis 4c). Figure 4 displayed significant relations between aspects of the home environment and attachment (Hypothesis 4d-1) and between the home environment and child self-control (Hypothesis 4d-2). The path between attachment at 24 months and self-control at 36 months indicated a small effect, even when controlling for other study variables (Hypothesis 4e-1). There were some differences between demographic groups, e. g., gender, economic groups (Hypothesis 41). However, limited variance within economic groups may have made it difficult to detect certain effects. Conclusion The results of data analyses indicated partial support for the hypotheses. Some findings related to global sensitivity were somewhat unexpected; also findings related to gender differences were unanticipated. These findings will be discussed in the final chapter. In addition, limitations and future directions for research will be included. 114 CHAPTER 5 DISCUSSION The previous chapter included results of analyses, tests of hypotheses, and findings related to parental efficacy, home environments, children’s attachment relationships, and child self-control at 3 years of age. This chapter will discuss these findings. The first section of this chapter presents findings in relation to the research questions from Chapter 1. Additional findings follow, including gender differences in developmental processes. A section addressing limitations of the study and suggestions for future research conclude the chapter. Research Questions The NICHD Study of Early Child Care investigated the relation between infant child care and the quality of attachment (NICHD, 1997). One of the goals of this dissertation was to extend their study by including dimensions of parental efficacy and later child outcomes. In addition, this dissertation tested the universal application of hypotheses across demographic groups. The first set of research questions focused on parental self-efficacy and its relation to attachment. Second, an investigation of child care quality partially replicated findings from the NICHD (1997) study. Third, predictors of attachment and self—control in early childhood are discussed. 115 Dimensions of Parental Efi‘r‘cacy 1. Can counterintuitive findings from earlier studies of the relation between parental efficacy and attachment security be explained by reexamining dimensions of parental efficacy, i.e., competence and control? 2. Does parental efficacy have different effects on parenting and attachment among different demographic groups, specifically, low-income and middle-to-high income families and African American and Caucasian families? A differentiation between the dimensions of parental eflicacy, perceived control and perceived competence, yielded interesting results. Although past research has referred to both dimensions as representative of parental efficacy, the results of this research indicate that they are two distinct parental beliefs. Perceived competence was associated with attachment classification as measured by the Strange Situation Procedure at 15 months, particularly among Afiican Americans; however, perceived control was not related to attachment classification. With the total sample, the correlations of perceived control and perceived competence with attachment security (Attachment Q—Set) at 24 months were equivalent, representing a small effect size. Yet, when the sample was broken into separate demographic groups, different associations emerged for perceived control and perceived competence as discussed below. Perceived competence. A review of the literature formd that past studies have produced counterintuitive findings related to perceived competence and attachment (e.g., Spieker & Booth, 1988). In these studies, mothers of children with insecure-avoidant attachment (Type A) felt that they were competent mothers. However, insecure-avoidant 116 attachment has been associated with harsh discipline and intrusive parenting. Children with insecure-avoidant attachments were more likely to have behavior problems in school than children with secure or insecure—resistant attachment classifications (Renken, Egeland, Marvinney, Mangelsdorf, & Sroufe, 1989; Waters, etal., 1979). Thus, the high, and in some studies, higher levels of perceived competence among mothers with children classified as insecure-avoidant were not explained by the theory of self-efficacy. One possible explanation of inconsistent effects is the way that parental efficacy is measured. Bandura believed that perceived competence should be measured by specific behaviors, e.g., I am competent at soothing my baby when he/she is upset. In contrast, the Sense of Competence scale used in this dissertation measures global feelings about parental competence, e.g., I feel capable and on top of things when I am caring for my baby. Yet, some researchers have found associations with global scales of parental competence (e.g., del Carmen, Pedersen, Huffman, & Bryan, 1993); others have found no effects with measures of specific behaviors (e.g., Corapci & Wachs, 2002). A recent study comparing the Sense of Competence scale with the Maternal Self-Efficacy Questionnaire, a domain-specific measure (Teti & Gelfand, 1991), found that only the domain-specific measure was a predictor of child outcome among White middle-class mothers. Although the literature suggests that domain-specific measures are preferable measures of competence, the purpose of this dissertation is not to compare domain- specific and global measures, but to examine why global measures of perceived competence are sometimes ineffective. In other studies, perceived competence has been associated with positive parenting and secure attachment. Findings in the current 117 dissertation suggest that a global measure of perceived competence may not be related to positive parenting and attachment security within all groups and cultures. In this dissertation, the strength of associations between global perceived competence and attachment differed between demographic groups. Researchers have speculated that the stronger associations with parental efficacy found among diverse groups may be related to greater variance in parental efficacy scores within disadvantaged groups (Coleman & Karraker, 2003). Yet, a test comparing variances between groups indicated that there was no significant difference in perceived competence scores between Afiican Americans and Caucasians or between low- and middle-to-high income families. Among low-income families, mothers who perceived themselves as competent when their children were one month old were more likely to have children who were securely attached at 24 months. However, this effect was not significant among higher income families. It is possible that risks associated with economic disadvantage make assets, such as a mother who has positive feelings about her parenting abilities, more essential to the child’s development of attachment security. In higher income families, other resources, such as quality child care, a supportive community, or a supportive father, may compensate for a mother who feels insecure in her parental role. Similarly, differential risks among African American and Caucasian families affect the hypothetical process of parental efficacy influencing parenting, which in turn influences attachment development. The bivariate correlation between perceived competence at one month and attachment security at 24 months was 0.24 among African Americans; the same correlation was .08 among Caucasians. According to 118 Bronfenbrenner’s (2000) theory, Afiican American or low-income families should have stronger links between perceived competence and attachment security than Caucasian or middle-class families. Parenting and other processes that directly affect the child may be more important to children who experience more risk. For example, families from middle-class homes are able to draw on other resources when a mother is depressed or neglectful of her parenting role. Neighborhood playgroups, a supportive school system, and medical insurance covering the cost of treatment are likely to be available to assist the family through difficult times. Thus, access to other environments that promote successful child outcomes may compensate somewhat for deficits in the home environment, and links between the home environment and child outcomes are theorized to be weaker among more advantaged groups. There were also differential associations of perceived competence and attachment classification at 15 months. In a regression of income, perceived competence, and parenting on attachment classification, only income was a significant predictor of attachment classification among Caucasians. Within the African American sample, mothers who were less sensitive and had children with insecure-avoidant classification appeared to think of themselves as competent parents. Although they were less responsive and sensitive to their children than mothers with secmely attached children, they perceived themselves to be equally competent. Mothers who had children with insecure-resistant classification did not report feeling competent as parents. This finding duplicates those of a study of attachment among at-risk families, with mothers who had insecure-avoidant children scoring as high on measures of 119 perceived competence as mothers of securely attached children, and mothers with insecure-resistant children being significantly lower on perceived competence (Spieker & Booth, 1988). Although their study did not directly explain the unexpected findings related to perceived competence, they did point out that mothers of avoidant children in at—risk families tend to idealize their relationships. Perhaps mothers of avoidant children in families disadvantaged by the social and economic structure of our society (i.e., racism) would also idealize their parenting abilities. Furthermore, the racial composition of their sample is not cited; thus, it is not possible to draw conclusions about effects of racism within their study. Analyses of the NICHD data also indicated that Afiican American families were rated as less sensitive or positive parents. Links between sensitivity and attachment classification at 15 months were only present among the Caucasian group. It should be noted that global measures of sensitivity were assessed by mostly White middle-class graduate students who were, in general, untrained to understand Afiican American parenting styles. Global sensitivity measures have been used with success for decades beginning with Ainsworth et al.’s (1978) classic attachment study, but it is only in recent years that issues related to observer bias have been raised. Although measurement methods may have been culturally inappropriate, it is also likely that differences in the general style of mother-child interaction existed between African Americans and Caucasians. It may be that African Americans place less of an emphasis on positive parenting and responsiveness, resulting in different links between perceived competence, parenting, and attachment. The literature suggests that Afiican Americans value parents who teach their children to survive in a challenging environment 120 (Murry, 2000). Afiican American parents who teach their children to be obedient and hard workers may be considered competent parents. Cultural differences contributing to variations in parenting style and motivations may explain differences in the relations among attachment and perceived competence among Afiican American and Caucasian parents. While positive attitudes and emotional security are certainly valued, Afiican American parents have been shown to approach discipline in a different way than Caucasian families (Murry, 2000; McLoyd, Cauce, Takeuchi, & Wilson, 2000). Thus, it may be considered preferable to parent in accordance with cultural imperatives, socializing Afiican American children to succeed within their environment. Perceived control. In a structural equation model of the total sample, perceived control was a significant predictor of attachment security as measured by the Attachment Q-Set, even when controlling for maternal depression and parenting practices. This indicated the importance of parents’ beliefs about their effects on child outcomes. Although processes within African American and Caucasian families were not modeled because of the higher attrition among African Americans, other analyses indicate some differences in process. In a bivariate correlation, perceived control was associated with attachment security among African American families (r = .26). This is particularly notable considering that perceived control was measured when the child was 1 month old; attachment security was measured when the child was 24 months. Among Caucasian families, perceived control was not associated with attachment security at 24 months (r = .06). This may be due to a differential variance in perceived control scores. African American mothers had a significantly higher variance in perceived control scores than Caucasians. 121 Another possible explanation is that a sense of control would be a stronger asset in a less predictable environment. Many African Americans who experience unfair treatment may not view their environment as something that they can control. One study found that Afiican Americans who perceive that they are unfairly treated due to racism passed teachings and messages about mistrust on to their children through racial socialization (Hughes & Johnson, 2001). The literature suggests that these teachings occur at a later developmental stage; however, it is possible that subtle non-verbal messages may occur in early childhood. Furthermore, among African Americans who reported higher levels of discrimination, linkages between stress and psychological problems and between psychological problems and mother-child interaction were stronger (Murry, Brown, Brody, Cutrona, & Simons, 2001). Thus, negative aspects of children’s environments had a stronger influence when mothers perceived their daily experiences as being affected by capricious discriminatory acts. Parents who perceive that they are in control of their firture and their children’s firture may use this perception as a positive coping mechanism. It is of interest that there were no significant group differences in the association of perceived control and attachment security at 24 months between low-income families and middle-to-high income families in the structural equation model or within bivariate correlations. However, there is a significant amount of difference in variance between scores of the two groups, with low-income families having more variance in levels of perceived control. In past research, effects of Afiican American status have been confounded with income effects (Murry, 2000). By viewing economic groups and racial groups separately, it is possible to make distinct inferences about Afiican American 122 families apart from their economic status. Although it was not possible to include comparisons of processes between racial groups using structural equation models due to the limited sample size, ad-hoc analyses (see Appendix B) assisted in disentangling factors of class and race. Even though the sample size was small for middle-to-high income African Americans (n = 16), t-tests indicated group differences when contrasted with middle-to-high income Caucasians. This robust finding suggests strong cultural effects that supercede class differences, and further research is certainly indicated. In sum, three factors may result in counter-intuitive effects related to parental efficacy and attachment. First, perceived control produced different effects than perceived competence. Second, in families with more risk factors, efficacy and parenting styles appear to be more strongly related to children’s outcome. For instance, a lack of perceived control among Afiican Americans may be more detrimental to their children’s environments than among groups who do not experience discrimination. Third, Afiican American mothers with insecure-avoidant children were likely to overestimate their parental competence in comparison with their parenting skills, as defined by the measure of positive parenting. A trend toward this effect was noted among the total sample, but not among Caucasian families. Child Care Quality 3. Does child care quality moderate the effects of maternal depression and parenting practices on mother-child attachment? An earlier study with these data found no main effects of child care quality or the amount of child care on attachment classification at 15 months (NICHD, 1997). Because 123 of these earlier results, only interactions were tested in this study. Tests indicated that child care quality was not a significant moderator of maternal depression and attachment. However, child care quality did moderate the relation between positive parenting and attachment. Although this dissertation used different measures of child care quality and mother-child interaction, results from the earlier NICHD (1997) study were replicated. Child care quality did not directly predict attachment security as measured by the Strange Situation Procedure at 15 months; however, mothers who chose good quality child care and behaved sensitively toward their children were more likely to have securely attached children. Bivariate correlations with child care quality in infancy suggest that a different approach to modeling may yield other results. Higher child care quality was significantly related to income, less maternal depression, sensitivity, positive/negative parenting, an enriched home environment, attachment security at 24 months as measured by the Attachment Q-Set, and children’s self control at 36 months (See Table 5). Other methods of modeling these data will be discussed in the section describing future research directions. Social-emotional Development in Early Chiltflrood 4. Are the data from the National Institute of Child Health and Development Study of Early Child Care consistent with the model linking parental efficacy, parenting practice, and children’s social-emotional outcomes as shown in Figure 2? 124 5. Does the model fit vary as a function of demographic status? For example, do the relations among the variables differ for low-income and middle-to-high income families? In general, the model shown in Figure 2 fit the data, suggesting that maternal perceived control and mater-ml depression predict parenting practices and attachment, which in turn predict children’s self-control. Models including marital conflict also showed goodness of fit, excepting for the model with girls only. Gender differences are discussed later in the chapter. Maternal characteristics. In accordance with the literature, maternal depression predicted more negative mother-child interaction. Mothers who were more depressed were less likely to provide an enriched home environment. Through parenting and the home environment, depression affected children’s later attachment security and self- control. There was also a small but significant direct effect between depression and children’s attachment security. Thus, depressed mothers were more likely to have children with insecure attachment, even when positive/negative mother-child interaction was controlled. Additional effects were found between perceived control, a dimension of parental efficacy, and study variables. As expected, perceived control was associated with depression. This association was substantially larger among low-income families. There was a small but significant direct effect of perceived control on attachment. Effects of perceived control on the home environment were generally not significant, except that middle-to-high income mothers with more perceived control were likely to provide a more enriched home environment. The effect size for the path between perceived control 125 and enrichment among middle-to-high income mothers was actually equivalent to the effect size for the same path among low-income mothers. Significance testing varied only due to sample size. Parenting and child characteristics. In turn, an enriched home environment was related to attachment. Mother-child interactions of positive and negative parenting also predicted attachment security. Many of these associations were reduced when the sample was divided into low-income and income families. A possible cause of reduced effects may be the lower variance within economic groups. An enriched home environment at 15 months was related to child self-control measured 1‘/2 years later at 36 months. The measure of enriched home environments included regular experiences, both formal and informal, that teach young children as well as the provision of toys and other materials for teaching children. It is possible that mothers who spend time teaching and playing with their children in developmentally appropriate ways also help them to develop self-control. Mothers who spend time teaching their children may enhance their social behaviors as well as assisting their cognitive development. As expected, attachment also predicted children’s self-control. A positive attachment relationship assists children in their behavioral regulation (Davies, Harold, Goeke-Murray, & Cummings, 2002). Marital Conflict The model including marital conflict indicated some minimal effects. Among middle-to-high income families but not low-income families, marital conflict was a significant addition to the model. The variance in marital conflict was, in fact, 126 significantly higher within the low-income group than among the middle-to-high income group. Thus, differential effects are not explained by a restricted range. There are other possible explanations for the limited effects of marital conflict within low-income families, besides differential variance. It may be that low-income mothers depend more on other family members and friends for support in raising their children than on their spouse. Men in low-income families may be less egalitarian in gender roles, if only because of the additional pressures to succeed in the role of breadwinner. Thus, marital conflict may have less of an effect on parenting and child outcome if the father is not actively engaged in childrearing. Although this study did not examine the effects of marital conflict within ethnic groups, ethnicity may be an important factor. Both gender roles and the effects of marital conflict differ within cultural context (McLoyd, Cauce, Takeuchi, & Wilson, 2000). For example, Afiican American men are more egalitarian in gender roles but not in attitudes towards women. It is also important to note that a higher proportion of African Americans are single in comparison with Caucasians, particularly among low-income families. In this study, 56% of the African American mothers were no longer living with the father of the child at 15 months old in comparison to only 9% of the Caucasians. Marital conflict was measured when the child was 1 month old; other measures were collected from 15 to 36 months later. Both cohabiting and married partners completed these measures. In high-income households, 2% of the fathers were no longer living at home at 15 months, but 38% of the fathers were no longer present at 15 months among low-income families. Hence, it may be that differences in low-income and high- income families related to the importance of partner conflict are a reflection of unstable 127 family structure, which is generally more prevalent in low-income households. Marital conflict that existed with the father of the child may not affect the home environment as much if the father is no longer present. Among middle-to-high income families, mothers with more perceived control were less likely to experience marital conflict. Mothers reporting depression were likely to experience more marital conflict. Marital conflict was related to negative parenting; marital conflict indirectly affects attachment through this path. Thus, expected associations existed between marital conflict and study variables, but only for higher- income families. In sum, differences in process between low-income families and middle-to-high income families existed in this sample. Although effects of perceived control on attachment did not differ between groups as expected, effects of maternal depression on negative parenting were stronger among low-income families. In turn, effects of parenting, particularly positive parenting and enrichment, on attachment and self-control were stronger among low-income families. Overall, most proximal processes had a stronger effect on child outcome within low-income families. ¥The importance of parenting practices to children living in low-income families suggests that parent education programs would be particularly beneficial for this group. Presently, family programs, such as home visiting forlowlincome families, arenet well- supported at a federal level in the United States. Government support for experimental demonstration programs has improved in the past decade, particularly with positive results coming fi'om certain programs (Olds & Korfrnacher, 1997). However, few parent education programs are available nationwide for families with children in the preschool 128 K2,. 4‘ f t ~ years. Previous research indicates that early childhood is the stage when family resources are most influential (Duncan & Brooks-Gum, 2000). Thus, parent education programs and programs that provide additional resoru'ces, such as healthcare, nutrition, and learning activities, need to find more avenues to reach young children before they enter school. Gender Differences A division of the sample by gender suggested different processes for boys and girls. The literature suggests that gender differences in behavior emerge during later years, when children are 4 or 5 years of age (Achenbach, Edelbrock, & Howell, 1987; Zahn-Waxler, Ianotti, Cummings, & Denham, 1990). Boys tend to have more externalizing behavior problems than girls in later years. Yet, some gender differences related to emotionality in infancy have been found, with boys being more negatively emotional during mother-child interaction than girls (Bralmgart-Rieker, Courtney, & Garwood, 1999). This study found that girls exhibited more self-control than boys at 36 months. In this dissertation, the Strange Situation Procedure at 15 months also favored girls; girls were significantly more likely to be securely attached than boys. In previous research, significant gender differences with respect to attachment quality are not often evident. Yet, a study of low-income African American families also found that boys were significantly less likely to be securely attached than girls (Barnett et al., 1998). This study measured attachment during the preschool years and not in infancy. Results from the present data indicated that attachment at 24 months was related to children’s self-control at 36 months for boys but not for girls. Other studies have 129 found that early attachment security and later behavioral expression varied by gender. A study of middle-class families found an association for boy infants but not for girl infants between insecure attachment and later psychopathology (Lewis, Feiring, McGuffog, & Jaskir, 1984). Another study of toddlers found that early attachment was related to later autonomy for girls; a similar association was not found for boys (Aber & Baker, 1990). Other significant gender differences include effects of negative parenting and marital conflict. First, the path between negative parenting and child self-control was significant for girls but not for boys. Social learning theory suggests that girls are more likely to model negative maternal behavior than boys (Crockenberg & Langrock, 2001). For example, if parents show lack of self-control in their behavior with their children, it is possible that girls are more likely to model that behavior. Thus, assuming that negative parenting, such as angry or aggressive behavior toward the child, represents low maternal self-control, girls would be more likely than boys to have lower self-control in response to viewing maternal uncontrolled behavior. In the present study, a model including marital conflict fit for boys but not for girls; other studies have found gender differences related to children’s reactions to marital conflict (e.g., Davies, Harold, Goeke-Morey, & Cummings, 2002). It is of interest that significant gender differences in attachment security have been noted among dual-earner households, but not among single-eamer households, with more negative effects present for boys (Belsky & Rovine, 1988; Chase-Lansdale & Owen, 1987). The oversampling of dual-earner households for this study may have exaggerated gender differences in comparison with the national population. Further study of gender differences and child care usage may clarify these questions. 130 Limitations Certain limitations are inherent in secondary data analysis. In this particular study, the diversity of measures and overall quality of measurement methods is above average. Yet, primary research allows for more focus on a particular area of interest. In this study, the use of partial measures rather than complete measures may have reduced reliability, particularly on the measures of perceived control and temperament. The use of the Early Infancy Temperament Questionnaire, which was under development at the time of the study, rather than an established measure made it difficult to reach adequate reliability. It would have been useful for the purposes of this study to have additional measures of perceived competence, such as the domain-specific Maternal Self-Efficacy Questionnaire. Although the total sample size is more than adequate, division of the sample into groups makes certain analyses difficult For example, the number of African American families (n = 176) was too small for an accurate analysis using structural equation modeling, especially considering attrition or missing data. An individual researcher collecting primary data could have oversampled Afiican American families in order to compare processes in African American and Caucasian families. The number of low- income families and middle class families was sufficient for two separate structural equation models; however, additional separate models comparing processes by child gender were not possible. The sample itself, although largely representative of demographics within the United States, is not a truly random sample. A purposeful sampling plan reflecting demographic diversity included mothers who planned to use child care full-time (60%) 131 or part-time (20%) and mothers who planned to stay at home (20%). The sample is limited to ten localities, mostly in the Eastern United States. Of greater importance, the sample excludes families living in unsafe neighborhoods, infants with perinatal problems requiring extensive hospitalization, school-age mothers, and mothers with insufficient English skills. In addition, those who declined to participate (42%) may have had more difficulties in their families than those who agreed to participate. Thus, the sample of low-income families is actually more advantaged than samples of low— income families in other national studies. The exclusion of high-risk families may have made it more difficult to detect differences between low-income and middle-to-high income economic groups. In addition to the exclusion of high-risk families, the NICHD Study of Early Child Care excluded high-risk child care settings. Exclusion was not necessarily an effect of predefined qualifications for the study; it appeared that child care settings that were not optimal did not participate. A comparison between nationally representative data on child care quality and the NICHD sample suggested that national child care quality is lower and more variable (N ICHD, 2000). Exclusion of low quality child care and high-risk families may have resulted in lower effect sizes of child care quality, assuming that quality of care would have stronger effects among higher-risk groups. Attrition was relatively low for this study; however, father data were missing at a much higher rate. Documentation from NTCHD (1999a) cites l-month father data as being collected at all sites; however, data were only available for 6 of the 10 sites. Furthermore, data were not randomly missing even within the 6 sites, with fathers from higher-risk families being less likely to participate. Although a separate study could be 132 made of father’s beliefs and involvement, it was not advisable to include father information in a study of the total sample due to the lack of consistent data collection. Future Directions Earlier studies from this data set found limited effects of child care quality on attachment security. However, there were significant interactions between parenting practices and child care quality on attachment. Although this study replicated the finding that child care quality combined with positive parenting predicted attachment security, it is possible that child care quality by itself is a predictor of mother-child attachment and child self-control. A structural equation model including only children in child care may answer questions about the significance of child care quality to children’s developing attachment relationships that may not be detected with the use of a regression model (Newcombe, 2003). Recent evidence suggests that significant amounts of time in child care during the early years is related to socioemotional maladjustment (NICHD, 2003). Additional effects of time in child care could also be investigated with this data set. Controls for income may artificially isolate the effects of child care (Newcombe, 2003). Fmther investigation of time in child care, attachment security, and behavior problems could use structural equation modeling to examine pathways between variables. Further study is also suggested to examine effects of child care quality and time in child care among children of color. Child care quality, as measured in this dissertation, was only available for 82 African American children. Limited analyses could certainly be performed with this sample size. Yet, the NICHD has limited 133 information that may be relevant to the effect of child care on African American children. Discrimination and segregation within the child care setting are not measured; ethnic identity and perceived discrimination would also be helpful measures to ascertain the effects of racial socialization. Additional primary studies are needed to further investigate children’s environments, including child care, and their meaning for children of color (Johnson, Jaeger, Randolph, Cauce, Ward, & NICHD, 2003). In this dissertation, dimensions of parental efficacy had different effects on family process among Afiican American families than among Caucasian families. Although it is possible to speculate about reasons for differential effects, further research is necessary to explain these differences. A study measuring ethnic identity, perceptions of discrimination, and their relation to parental efficacy among families of color may assist in understanding how these processes work. Certainly, more work could be done among Latino, Asian, and Native American families to gain further cross-cultural perspectives. X Unexpected findings related to gender differences could be investigated with ommwathpfimary research. It wouldbe particularlyusefill to investigate father involvement when studying gender differences with self-control. Earlier research has indicated that fathers play a unique role in children’s development, particularly in the development of self-control (Braungart—Rieker, Garwood, Powers, & Wang, 2001). Parenting styles of both fathers and mothers may differ between ethnicities; hence, it would be useful to include diverse groups in this study. It is of interest that we found few effects of perceived control on parenting itself, although perceived control was associated with child outcomes. One study of mostly 134 middle—class Caucasian families found that neither global perceived competence or domain-specific perceived competence was related to parenting, yet a domain-specific measure of perceived competence was related to toddler behavior (Coleman & Karraker, 2003). Other studies have found more effects of perceived control on parenting among low-income mothers than among higher-income mothers; however, these studies may have confounded ethnicity with income. Further research among diverse groups may explain these effects. Conclusion In summary, findings indicated N" T environments are predictive of attachment security and children’s self-control. Separate that parental efficacy and children’s proximal 2W_ descriptive analyses of African American and Caucasian families added to our understanding of children’s social-emotional development within different contexts. In particular, findings regarding parental efficacy indicated distinct effects within Afi'ican American and Caucasian groups. Furthermore, the traditional measurements of sensitivity and attachment indicated possible cultural differences in the ways and meanings of parenting. Dimensions of perceived competence and perceived control also produced different effects on parenting style and child outcomes. Linkages between parenting and child outcomes were generally stronger among low-income groups than middle-to-high income groups, suggesting that proximal processes have a greater effect among children living in disadvantaged families than among children with more resources. Findings on gender, ethnicity, and class raised interesting questions that warrant further investigation. In sum, this dissertation added to our understanding of 135 parental efficacy and its relation to various aspects of children’s environments, attachment security, and child self—control within diverse demographic groups. 136 APPENDICES 137 APPENDIX A MEASURES 138 Sense of Competence Subscale 1. When my baby came home from the hospital, I had doubtful feelings about my ability to handle being a parent. 2. Being a parent is harder than I thought it would be. 3. I feel capable and on top of things when I am caring for my baby. 4. I can’t make decisions without help. 5. I have had many more problems caring for my baby than I expected. 6. I enjoy being a parent. 7. I feel that I am successful most of the time when I try to get my baby to do or not do something. 8. Since I brought this baby home from the hospital, I find that I am not able to take care of this baby as well as I thought I could. I need help. 9. I often have the feeling that I cannot handle things very well. 139 Perceived Control Subscale 8. 9. . If a child has tantrums, no matter what you try you might as well give up. My baby influences the number of friends I have. I have often found that when it comes to my child, what is going to happen, will happen. My baby’s behavior is sometimes more that I can handle. Being a good parent often depends on being lucky enough to have a good baby. When something goes wrong with my baby there is little I can do to correct it. It is often easier to let my baby have his/her way than to put up with a tantrum. My baby does not control my life. I feel like what happens in my life is mostly determined by my baby. 10. Fate was kind to me; if I had had a bad baby I don’t know what I would have done. 11. What I do has little effect on my baby’s behavior. 12. I’m just one of those parents who happened to have a good child. 13. Sometimes I feel my baby’s behavior is hopeless. 14. My life is chiefly controlled by my baby. 140 Center for Epidemiological Studies — Depression 1. I was bothered by things that usually don’t bother me. 2. I felt that everything I did was an effort. 3. I felt I was just as good as other people. 4. I had trouble keeping my mind on what I was doing. 5. I felt sad. 6. I felt fearful. 7. I felt lonely. 8. I had crying spells. 9. I talked less than usual. 10. My sleep was restless. 11. I enjoyed life. 12. I felt that I could not shake off the blues even with the help of my family/friends. 13. I thought my life had been a failure. 14. I was happy. 15. I could not get “going”. 16. I felt hopeful about the future. 17. People were unfriendly to me. 18. I did not feel like eating; my appetite was poor. 19. I felt depressed. 20. I felt that people dislike me. 141 Marital Conflict Subscale 1. How often do you and your partner argue with one another? 2. To what extent do you try to change things about your partner that bother you (e. g. behaviors, attitudes, etc.) ? 3. How confused are you about your feelings toward your partner? 4. How much do you think or worry about losing some of your independence by being involved with your partner? 5. How often do you feel angry or resentful toward your partner? 6. How ambivalent or unsure are you about continuing in the relationship with your partner? 7. To what extent do you feel that your partner demands or requires too much of your time and attention? 8. To what extent do you feel “trapped” or pressured to continue in this relationship? 9. When you and your partner argue, how serious are the problems or arguments? 142 APPENDIX B AD-HOC ANALYSES OF INCOME AND RACE 143 .28. vac: no. 2 9.6 $2 523230 mo. 36 a: 3.6. 3.2 mm... 3 2282.205 582.22 35822055 .3. 2 «.2..» an 2 528250 8. m2. 3. ma. 2- 222.2 8.22 82 5°55 §E< Hosanna hm. «Om an 2 52.3030 mm. 2.”. an. 22 2 .2- we. mmd 82 22823:? 2.82.2.2 "2228222825 $6 2.2 2 232 528030 3. ma. 2.. mm. 22. 222.2 cm 5828252 5854‘ ”.88 n2 ammo 22. v02 32 523250 222. 32 2a. 2 2.- 8.2. 3.2 02 2 22.823222 2282...: ”.88 v.2 ammo 9.. mm. m cm 2 523250 co. Sam 2m. 32 on. 222.24 2 2 502.55 2282.22 ”002282288 2.928.682 2 m. 2.9m cm 2 523250 S. pen on. em. an. 24m n 22 2232.85 2282.22 ”2888 2828.52 .flw w222|a>d~ ram use: QM :2 2 o2naum> llldlqmooeetuar .20 2282 m Jana—822 .2 22 m m. 30 fifimauafiu 3:3 :QUNLNEV 23.9% V. NEOSQJSQN 2NMENQ hmufifimkfiap 9 mm 2an 144 .28. V Q2... 2.. 2 N.- m m 2 52.8230 .:.. on.» 2.22. Q2- 3. on: 2.2. Serena §E< ”20.52.8225 om. em. 2222 528230 no. 8. 8. www- om. m 2. ma 2282.62.22 582.2,: 38822082 220m 362 mm 2328030 ea. 86 222. SN- 234 21.2 ma 582.2222 502.5. 52232. as 225 cc. 2 and an 2 523250 on. 2.28. No. and n 2 .2 and an 2282.225, §E< H8222228 282.32 32 and 32 23286st 22a. 8. mm. mm: v.02 214 3 502.2222 2282.223. ”E2339: .3 e203 cm. 2 m~.n 32 523230 R. mm. 8. and- 32.2 3.2. 3 2282.55 §E< $265.28 3282 am .4352 1am can: a. 2 a czar; 3% flag 9.2802 mm 033. 145 .28. V Re: mm. 3.6 now 523250 .:.... a2 .2 no. and- 3.2 2.2.6 2 502.5223 §E< ”Bogotcm on. 2 and n 2 w 2823230 22. no. 222. no. 2- 2.02 and 2 2232.05 §o£< Essex 3. and n 2 w 5230.50 mm. 2:. M2. 32- Nm. mnN 2 582.282 §E< ”2823222825 36 3.6 new 528260 co. 9: 2 2. 20.2 no.» 56 2 2282.205 2.82.2.2 ”.88 n2 Q-mmo $26 32.2. awn 2823250 3.. on. 222. 3.2 36 NW2: 2 502.232 §E< ”.88 0.2 O-mmo mm. 8.2. own 522830 2. 2mm mm. 3.. ov. 3.». 2 2232.225. §E< 6882288 2333.292 we. and am 528250 3. mm. an. 3.. mv. 3.2m 02 532.2222 2.88222 "22:28 2.920292 .3 I lacs 5 raw Elsi no. :2 2 23%|. > alone-jg. a: m a: an 22% m e. 3e agencaeb 236 toutoEV 58.52.. 2:822 fiME-S-eESE $953 $33.2me Q2233» 238 entomEWe b.2233? SN 28$ mm 2an 146 .28. V use... 2.22.. m 2. «9. 528030 2.N. mm. 2 8. m m.- 2.. mm: m 2 582.5252 §E< ”20bcoo-22om om. mm. 229. 523230 mm. 2.2 22. 20.2- 2. mm. 2 582.2222 §E< “22225208222 31. 09mm 3m 523250 cm. 8. .:.... and- 36 3.62 N2 582.2252 §oE< 522.32. 88 226 3. o2.m nan 528250 3. cum 8. 2.28 ea. $.m o2 2.82.2222. 22822:, 2202—2228 282.32 2 a. and non 2825830 we. 52. 2.2. 5.2- mo. 86 2 532.2222. 50$? 553mg .20 V23 an. m2..n non 282.8250 mm. 3.2 8. mm; mm. 2.96 n 2 502.2222 §E< ”922228.322 322282 am 834.82 ram oat-a: mm :2 a azaleal> llhqlm-aeaza 223. m ease 8 asap 147 REFERENCES 148 Aber, J ., & Baker, A. (1990). Secruity of attachment in toddlerhood: Modifying assessment procedures for joint clinical and research purposes. In M. Greenberg, D. Cicchetti, & E. Cummings (Eds), Attachment in the Preschool Years: Theory, research, and intervention (pp. 427-460). Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. Abbott-Shim, M. & Sibley, A. (1992). Assessment profile for early childhood programs: Research version Atlanta, GA: Quality Assist, Inc. Abidin, R. R. (1986). Parenting stress index. Charlottesville, VA: Pediatric Psychology Press. Achenbach, T. M., Edelbrock, C., & Howell, C. (1987). Empirically-based assessment of the behavioral/emotional problems of 2-3 year old children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 15, 629-650. Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of attachment: A psychological stuay of the strange situation. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. \/ Ardelt, M., & Eccles, J. S. (2001). Effects of mother’s parental efficacy beliefs and promotive parenting strategies on inner-city youth. Journal of Family Issues, 22, 944-972. Bandura, A. (1982). Self-efficacy mechanism in human agency. American Psychologist, 3 7, 122-147. \/ Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A social cognitive theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. Bandura, A. (1989). Regulation of cognitive processes through perceived self-efficacy. Developmental Psychology, 25, 729-735. Bandura, A. (1994). Self-efficacy. In V. S. Ramachaudran (Ed), Encyclopedia of human behavior (V 01. 4, pp. 71-81). New York: Academic Press. Bandura, A. (1995). Self-eflicacy in changing societies. New York: Cambridge University Press. Bandura, A., Barbaranelli, C., Caprara, G. V., & Pastorelli, C. (1996). Multifaceted impact of self-efficacy on academic functioning. Child Development, 67, 1206- 1222. Barnett, D., Kidwell, S., & Leung, K. (1998). Parenting and preschooler attachment among low-income urban African American families. Child Development, 69, 1657-1671. 149 Belsky, J. (1999). Child psychology: A Wk of contemporary issues. Philadelphia, PA: Psychology Press. Belsky, J ., & Isabella, R A. (1988). Maternal, infant, and social-contextual determinants of attachment security. In J. Belsky & T. Nezworski (Eds.), Clinical implications of attachment (pp. 41-94). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Belsky, J ., Lang, M. & Rovine, M. (1985). Stability and change in marriage across the transition to parenthood: A second study. Journ_a1 of Marriage and the Family. 47. 855-865. Belsky, J., & Rovine, M. (1988). Nonmaternal care in the first year of life and the security of infant-parent attachment. Child Development, 59, 157-167. Bentler, P. M., & Bonett, D. G. (1980). Significance tests and goodness of fit in the analysis of covariance structures. Psychological Bulletin, 88, 588-606. Bornstein, M. H. (2002). Handbook of parenting. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbamn. Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss, Vol. 1: Attachment. New York: Basic Books. Bowlby, J. (1988). A secure base: Parent-child attachment and healthy human development. New York: Basic Books. Braiker, H. & ey, H. (1979). Conflict in the development of close relationships. In - . urgess and T. Huston (Eds.), Social exchange and developing relationships. New York: Academic Press. Braungart, J. M., & Stifter, C. A. (1991). Regulation of negative reactivity during the strange situation: temperament and attachment in 12-month-old infants. Infant Behavior and Development, 1 4, 349-364. Braungart-Rieker, J., Courtney, S., & Garwood, M. M. (1999). Mother- and father- infant attachment: Families in context. Journal of Family Psychology, 13, 535- 553. . Braungart-Rieker, J ., Garwood, M. M., Powers, B. P., & Notaro, P. C. (1998). Infant affect and affect regulation during the still-face paradigm with mothers and fathers: The role of infant characteristics and parental sensitivity. Developmental Psychology, 34, 1428-1437. Braungart-Rieker, J. Garwood, M. M., Powers, B. P., & Wang, X. (2001). Parental sensitivity, infant affect, an affect regulation: Predictors of later attachment. Child Development, 72, 252-270. 150 Brazelton, T. B. (1983). Assessment techniques for enhancing infant development. In J. Call, E. Galenaon, & R. Tyson (Eds.), Frontiers of infant psychiatry. New York: Basic Books. Bretherton, I. (1985). Attachment theory: retrospect and prospect. In I. Bretherton & E. Waters (Eds.) Growing points of attachment theory and research. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 50 (1-2, Serial No. 209). Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The ecology of human development: Experiments by nature and design. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Bronfenbrenner, U. (1992). Ecological systems theory. In R. Vasta (Ed), Six theories of child development (pp. 187-249). London: Jessica Kingsley Publishers, Ltd. Bronfenbrenner, U. (1999). Environments in developmental perspective: Theoretical and operational models. In S. L. Friedman & T. D. Wachs (Eds.), Measuring environment across the life span: Emerging methods and concepts. (pp. 3-2 8). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Bronfenbrenner, U. (2000). Ecological systems theory. In A. E. Kazdin, (Ed.) Encyclopedia of psychology, Vol. 1. London: Oxford University Press. Bronfenbrenner, U., & Morris, P. A. (1998). The ecology of developmental processes. In W. Damon (Series Ed.) & R. M. Lerner (V 01. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 1. Theory. (5"I ed., pp. 993-1028). New York: Wiley. Browne, M. W., & Cudeck, R. (1993). Alternative ways of assessing model fit. In K. A. Bollen & J. S. Long (Eds.), Testing structural equation models (pp. 136-162). Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Bugental, D. B., Blue, J ., & Cruzcosa, M. (1989). Perceived control over caregiving outcomes: Implications for child abuse. Developmental Psychology, 25, 532-539. Bugental, D. B., & Shenum, W. A. (1984). “Difficult” children as elicitors and targets of adult communication patterns: An attributional-behavioral transactional analysis. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 49(], Serial No. 205). Caldwell, B. M., & Bradley, R. H. (2000). The home inventory for the observation of the environment. Little Rock, AK: University of Arkansas at Little Rock. Calkins, S. D. (1994). Origins and outcomes of individual differences in emotion regulation. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 59 (1 - 2, Serial No. 240). Calkins, S. & Fox, N. (1992). The relations among infant temperament, security of attachment, and behavioral inhibition at twenty-four months. Child Development, 63, 1456-1472. 151 Campis, L. K., Lyman, R. D., & Prentice-Dunn, S. (1986). The Parental Locus of Control Scale: Development and validation. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 15, 260-267. Carey, W. B., & McDevitt, S. C. (1978). Revision of the infant temperament questionnaire. Pediatrics, 61, 735-739. Casady, M. A., Diener, M., Isabella, R., & Wright, C. (2001, April). Attachment security among families in poverty: Maternal, child, and contextual characteristics. Poster session presented at the biennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Minneapolis, MN. Cassidy, J. (1994). Emotion regulation: Influences of attachment relationships. In N. A. Fox (Ed.), Emotion regulation: Behavioral and biological considerations. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development (288-249). Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Cassidy, J ., & Shaver, P. R. (1999). Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications. New York: Guilford Press. Chase-Lansdale, P. L., & Owen, M. T. (1987). Maternal employment in a family context: Effects on infant-mother and infant-father attachments. Child Development, 58, 1505-1512. Cicchetti, D., & Toth, S. (1990). Developmental Perspectives on Depression. Rochester, NY: University of Rochester Press. Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112, I, 155-159. Cohn, J ., & Campbell, S. B. (1992). Influence of maternal depression on infant affect regulation. In D. Cicchetti and S. L. Toth (Eds.), Developmental perspectives on depression (pp. 103-130). Rochester, NY: University of Rochester Press. \/ Coleman, P. K., & Karraker, K. H. (1997). Self-efficacy and parenting quality: Findings and future applications. Developmental Review, 18, 47-85. \/ Coleman, P. K., & Karraker, K. H. (2003). Maternal self-efficacy beliefs, competence in parenting, and toddlers' behavior and developmental status. Infant Mental Health Journal, 24, 126-148. / Corapci, F ., & Wachs, T. D. (2002). Does parental mood or efficacy mediate the influence of environmental chaos upon parenting behavior? Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 48, 182-201. 152 Crittenden, P. M. (2000). Introduction. In P. M. Crittenden and A. H. Claussen (Eds.) The organization of attachment relationships: Maturation, culture, and context. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Crnic, K. A., Greenberg, M. T., & Slough, N. M. (1986). Early stress and social support influences on mothers’ and high-risk infants’ functioning in late infancy. Infant Mental Health Journal, 7, 19-33. Crockenberg, S. (1981). Infant irritability, mother responsiveness, and social influences on the security of mother-infant attachment. Child Development, 52, 857-865. Crockenberg, S. B., & Langrock, A. (2001). The role of specific emotions in children’s responses to interparental conflict: A test of the model. Journal of Family Psychology, 15, 163-182. Cummings, E. M., & Davies, P. (1996). Emotional security as a regulatory process in normal development and the development of psychopathology. Development and Pscyhopathology, 8, 123-139. Davies, P. T., Harold, G. T., Goeke-Morey, M. C., & Cummings, E. M. (2002). Child emotional security and interparental conflict. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 6 7(3, Serial No. 270). De Wolff, M., & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (1997). Sensitivity and attachment: A meta- analysis on parental antecedents of infant attachment. Child Development, 68, 571-591 . Deater-Deckard, K., Dodge, K. A., Bates, J. E., Pettit, G. S. (1996). Physical discipline among African American and European American mothers: Links to children’s externalizing behaviors. Developmental Psychology, 32, 1065-1072. del Carmen, R., Pedersen, F. A., Huffman, L. C., & Bryan, Y. E. (1993). Dyadic distress management predicts subsequent security of attachment. Infant Behavior and Development, 16, 131-147. Dempster, A. P., Laird, N. M., & Rubin, D. B. (1977). Maximum likelihood fi'orn incomplete data via the EM algorithm. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society, Series B 39, 1-38. Deutsch, F. M., Ruble, D. N., Fleming, A., Brooks-Gunn, J., & Stangor, G. S. (1988). Information seeking and maternal self-definition during the transition to motherhood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55, 420-431. Diener, M., Nievar, M. A., & Wright, C. (2003). Attachment security between disadvantaged young children and their mothers: Associations with maternal, child and contextual characteristics. Merrill Palmer Quarterly, 49, 154-182. 153 Donovan, W., & Leavitt, L. (1989). Maternal self-efficacy and infant attachment: Integrating physiology, perceptions, and behavior. Child Development, 60, 460- 472. Donovan, W. L., Leavitt, L. A., & Walsh, R. O. (1997). Cognitive set and coping strategy affect mothers’ sensitivity to infant cries: A signal detection approach. Child Development, 68, 760-772. Donovan, W. L., Leavitt, L. A., & Walsh, R. O. (2000). Maternal illusory control predicts socialization strategies and toddler compliance. Developmental Psychology, 36, 1-10. Dumka, L. E., Stoerzinger, H. D., Jackson, K. M., & Roosa, M. W. (1996). Examination of the cross-cultural and cross-language equivalence of the Parenting Self-Agency Measure. Family Relations, 45, 216-222. Duncan, G. J ., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (2000). Family poverty, welfare reform, and child development. Child Development, 71, 188-196. F owles, D. C., & Kochanska, G. (2000). Temperament as a moderator of pathways to conscience in children: The contribution of electrodermal activity. Psychophysiology, 3 7, 788-795. French, J ., Rodgers, W., & Cobb, S. (1974). Adjustment as person-environment fit. In G. Cochlo, D. Harnberg, & J. Adams (Eds.), Coping and adaptation. New York: Basic. Gauthier, Y. (2003). Infant mental health as we enter the third millennium: Can we prevent aggression? Infant Mental Health Journal, 24, 296-308. Gjerde, P. F. (2001). Attachment, culture, and amae. American Psychologist, 56, 826- 827. Goldberg, W., & Easterbrooks, M. (1984). Role of marital quality in toddler development. Developmental Psychology, 20, 504-514. Goldsmith, H. H., & Alansky, J. A. (1987). Maternal and infant temperamental predictors of attachment: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 55, 805-816. Goldsmith, H. H., Buss, A. H., Plomin, R., Rothbart, M. K., Thomas, A., Chess, 8., Hinde, R. A., & McCall, R. R. (1987). Roundtable: What is temperament? Four approaches. Child Development, 58, 505-529. 154 Gondoli, D., & Silverberg, S. (1997). Maternal emotional distress and diminished responsiveness: The mediating role of parenting efficacy and parental perspective taking. Developmental Psychology, 33, 861 -868. Gonzales, N. A., Cauce, A. M., Friedman, R. J., & Mason, C. A. (1996). Family, peer, and neighborhood influences on academic achievement among African-American adolescents: One-year prospective effects. American Journal of Community Psychology, 24, 365-387. Grossman, K., Grossman, K. E., Spangler, G., Suess, G., & Uzner, L. (1985). Maternal sensitivity and newborns’ orientation responses as related to quality of attachment in northern Germany. In I. Bretherton & E. Waters (Eds.) Growing points in attachment theory and research. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 50(1-2, Serial No. 209). Grusec, J. E., Hastings, P., & Mammone, N. (1994). Parenting cognitions and relationship schemas. In W. Damon (Series Ed.) & J. G. Smetana (V 01. Ed.), Beliefs about parenting: Origins and developmental implications. New Directions for Child Development, 66, 5-20. San Francisco: Josey-Bass. Hamilton, S., & Luster, T. (2003). Ecological systems theory. In J. M. Miller, R. M. Lerner, L. B. Schiarnberg, Anderson (Eds). Human ecology: An encyclopedia of children, families, communities, and environments. Santa Barbara, CA: ABC- Clio. Harkness, S., & Super, C. M. (1992). Parental ethnotheories in action. In I. E. Sigel, A. V. McGillicuddy-Delisi, & J. J. Goodnow (Eds.). Parental belief systems. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Harris, J. R. (2002). Beyond the nurture assumption: Testing hypotheses about the child's environment. In J. G. Borkowski & S. L. Rarney (Eds.), Parenting and the child's world: Influences on academic, intellectual, and social-emotional development. Monographs in parenting. (pp. 3-20). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Harter, S. (1978). Effectance motivation reconsidered: Toward a developmental model. Human Development, 21, 34-64. Hastings, R. P., & Brown, T. (2002). Behavior problems of children with autism, parental self-efficacy, and mental health. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 107, 222-232. Hill, J ., Fonagy, P., Safier, E., & Sargent, J. (2003). The ecology of attachment in the family. Family Process, 42, 205-221. Hinde, R. A., & Stevenson-Hinde, J. (1990). Biological, cultural, and individual desiderata. Human Development, 33, 62-72. 155 Hoath, F. E., & Sanders, M. R. (2002). A feasibility study of enhanced group triple P - Positive Parenting Program for Parents of Children with Attention- deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. Behavior Change, 19, 191 -206. Hofferth, S. L., Smith, J ., McLoyd, V. C., & Finkelstein, J. (2000). Achievement and behavior among children of welfare recipients, welfare leavers, and low-income single mothers. Journal of Social Issues, 5 6, 747-774. Hosmer, D. W., & Lemeshow, S. (1989). Applied logistic regression. New York: Wiley. Hughes, D., & Johnson, D. (2001). Correlates in children’s experiences of parents’ racial socialization behaviors. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 63, 981- 985. Isabella, R. A. (1993). Origins of attachment: Maternal interactive behavior across the first year. Child Development, 64, 605-621. Isabella, R. A., Belsky, J ., & von Eye, A. (1989). Origins of infant-mother attachment: An examination of interactional synchrony during the infant’s first year. Developmental Psychology, 25, 12-21. Izard, C. E., Haynes, O. M., Chisholm, G., & Baak, K. (1991). Emotional determinants of infant-mother attachment. Child Development, 62, 906-917. Johnson, D., Jaeger, E., Randolph, S. M., Cauce, A. M., Ward, J ., & NICHD Early Child Care Research Network (2003). Studying the effects of early child care experiences on the development of children of color in the United States: Toward a more inclusive research agenda. Child Development, 74, 1227-1244. Johnston, C., & Mash, E. J. (1989). A measure of parenting satisfaction and efficacy. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 18, 167-175. Kagan, J. (1998). Biology and the child. In W. Damon (Series Ed.) & N. Eisenberg (V 01. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 111. Social, emotional, and personality development (5th ed., pp. 177-235). New York: Wiley. Kart-Morse, R., & Wiley, M. S. (1997). Ghosts fiom the nursery: Tracing the roots of violence. New York: Atlantic Monthly Press. Klein, L. (1998). Path analysis. In L. G. Grimm and P. R. Yamold (Eds.) Reading and understanding multivariate statistics (pp. 65-96). Washington, D. C.: American Psychological Association. Kline, R B. (1998). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling. New York: Guilford. 156 Kochanska, G. (1998). Mother-child relationship, child fearfulness, and emerging attachment: A short-term longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 3 4, 480-490. Kochanska, G., & Knaack, A. (2003). Effortfirl control as a personality characteristic of young children: Antecedents, correlates, and consequences. Journal of Personality, 71, 1087-11 12. Kochanska, G., Murray, K. T., & Harlan, E. T. (2000). Effortful control in early childhood: Continuity and change, antecedents, and implications for social development. Developmental Psychology, 3 6, 220-232. Kondo-Ikemura, K. (2001). Insufficient evidence. American Psychologist, 5 6, 825-826. Levine, R.A., Dixon S., Levine, S., Richman, A., Liederrnan, P.H., Keefer, C., Brazelton, T. (1994). Child care and culture: Lessons fiom Afi'ica. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Lewis, M., Feiring, C., McGuffog, C., & Jaskir, J. (1984). Predicting psychopathology in six-year-olds from early social relations. Child Development, 55, 123-136. Little, R. J. A., & Rubin, D. B. (1987). Statistical analysis with missing data. New York: Wiley. Little, R. J, & Rubin, D. B. (1989). The analysis of social science data with missing values. Sociological Methods and Research, 18, 292-326. Londerville, S., & Main, M. (1981). Security of attachment, compliance, and maternal training methods in the second year of life. Developmental Psychology, 1 7, 289- 299. Lovejoy, M. C., Verda, M. R., & Hays, C. E. (1997). Convergent and discriminant validity of measures of parenting efficacy and control. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 26, 366-376. Ludbrook, J. (1998). Multiple comparison procedures updated Clinical and Experimental Pharmacolog and Physiology, 25, 1032-1037. Luster, T., & Kain, E. (1987). The relation between family context and perceptions of parental efficacy. Early Child Development and Care, 29, 201-311. Luster, T., & Rhoades, K. (1989). The relation between child-rearing beliefs and the home environment in a sample of adolescent mothers. Family Relations, 38, 317-322. 157 Lyons-Ruth, K. (1992a). Maternal depressive symptoms, disorganized infant-mother attachment relationships and hostile-aggressive behavior in the preschool classroom: A prospective longitudinal view from infancy to age five. In D. Cicchetti & S. Toth (Eds.), Developmental Perspectives on Depression (pp. 131- 171). Rochester, NY: University of Rochester Press. Lyons-Ruth, K. (1992b). Maternal depression. In D. Cicchetti & S. Toth (Eds.), Developmental Perspectives on Depression Rochester, NY: University of Rochester Press. Lyons-Ruth, K. (1996). Attachment relationships among children with aggressive behavior problems: The role of disorganized early attachment patterns. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 64, 64-73. Machida, S., Taylor, A. R., & Kim, J. (2002). The role of maternal beliefs in predicting home learning activities in head start families. Family Relations, 51, 176-184. Main, M., & Solomon, J. (1990). Procedures for identifying infants as disorganized/disoriented during the Ainsworth strange situation. In M. Greenberg, D. Cicchetti, & E. M. Cummings (Eds.) Attachment in the preschool years: Theory, research, and intervention (pp. 121-160). Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. Mangelsdorf, S., Gunnar, M., Kestenbaum, R., Lang, S., & Andreas, D. (1990). Infant proneness-to-distress temperament, maternal personality, and mother-infant attachment: Associations and goodness of fit. Child Development, 61, 820-831. Martins, C., & Gaffan, E. A. (2000). Effects of early maternal depression on patterns of infant-mother attachment: A meta-analytic investigation. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 41, 73 7-746. Mash, E. J., & Johnston, C. (1983). The prediction of mothers’ behaviors with their hyperactive children during play and task situation. Child and Family Behavior Therapy, 5, 1-14. McLoyd, V. C., Cauce, A. M., Takeuchi, D., & Wilson, L. (2000). Marital processes and parental socialization in families of color: A decade review of research. Journal of Marriage and Family, 62, 1070-1093. Medoff-Cooper, B., Carey, W. B., & McDevitt, S. C. (1993). Early Infancy Temperament Questionnaire. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 14, 230-235. Murry, V. M. (2000). Challenges and experiences of Black American families. In P. C. McKenry & S. J. Price (Eds.), Families and change: Coping with stressful events. (2"d ed., pp. 333-358). 158 Murry, V. M., Brown, P. A., Brody, G. H., Cutrona, C. E., & Simons, R. L. (2001). Racial discrimination as a moderator of the links among stress, maternal psychological functioning, and family relationships. Journal of Marriage and Family, 63, 915-926. Newcombe, N. S. (2003). Some controls control too much. Child Development, 74, 1050-1052. NICHD Early Child Care Research Network (1997). The effects of infant child care on infant-mother attachment security: Results of the NICHD Study of Early Child Care. Child Development, 68, 860-879. NICHD Study of Early Child Care (1999a). Phase I Instrument Document. http://secc.rti.org= NICHD Early Child Care Research Network (1999b). Child care and mother-child interaction in the first 3 years of life. Developmental Psychology, 35, 1399-1413. NICHD Early Child Care Research Network (2000). Characteristics and quality of child care for toddlers and preschoolers. Journal of Applied Developmental Sciences, 4, 116-135. NICHD Early Child Care Research Network (2001). Child care and family predictors of preschool attachment and stability from infancy. Developmental Psychology, 3 7, 847-862. NICHD Early Child Care Research Network (2003). Does amount of time spent in child care predict socioemotional adjustment during the transition to kindergarten? Child Development, 74, 976-1005. NICHD Early Child Care Research Network, & Duncan, G. J. (2003). Modeling the impacts of child care quality on children’s preschool cognitive development. Child Development, 74, 1454-1475. Nievar, M. A., & Brophy-Herb, H. (2003, April). Parental Eflicacy and School Readiness for Kids: What Are the Connections? Symposium presented at the Biennial Conference of the Society for Research in Child Development, Tampa, FL. Ogbu, J. (1985). A cultural ecology of competence among inner-city blacks. In M. B. Spencer, G. K. Brookins, & W. R. Allen (Eds.), Beginnings: The social and aflective development of black children. Lawrence Erlbaum: Hillsdale, NJ. Olds, D. (1997). The prenatal/early infancy project: Fifteen years later. In G. Albee and T. Gullotta (Eds.), Primary prevention works. Issues in Children ’s and Families’ Lives, 6. (pp. 41-67). Sage Publications: Thousand Oaks, CA. 159 Olds, D., & Korfmacher, J. (1997). The evolution of a program of research on prenatal and early childhood home visitation: Special issue introduction. Journal of Community Psychology, 25, 1-7. Ozer, E. M., & Bandura, A. (1990). Mechanisms governing empowerment effects: A self-efficacy analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 472- 486. Papoulis, A. (1990). Probability and Statistics. Pearson Higher Education. Upper Saddle River, NJ. Parke, R. D. (2002). Fathers and families. In M. H. Bomstein (Ed), Handbook of parenting: Vol. 3: Being and becoming a parent. (pp. 27-73). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Pederson, D. R., & Moran, G. (1996). Expressions of the attachment relationship outside of the Strange Situation. Child Development, 6 7, 915-927. Peterson, L., Farmer, J ., & Kashani, J. (1990). Parental injury prevention endeavors: A function of health beliefs? Health Psychology, 9, 177-191. Polaha, J. A. (1999). The relationship between physical discipline and child behavior problems: A study of group differences. Dissertation Abstracts International, 59 (08), 451 1B. Posada, G., Gao, Y., Wu, F ., Posada, R., Tascon, M., Schoelmelich, A., Sagi, A., Kondo-Ikemura, K., Ylaland, W., & Synnevaag, B. (1995). The secure-base phenomenon across cultures: Children's behavior, mother's preferences, and experts' concepts. In E. Waters, B. Vaughn, G. Posada, & K. Kondo-Ikemura (Eds.), Caregiving, cultural, and cognitive perspectives on secure-base behavior and working models: New growing points of attachment theory and research (pp.27-48). Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 60 (2-3, Serial No. 244). Posada, G., Jacobs, A., Richmond, M. K., Carbonell, O. A., Alzate, G., Bustamante, M. R., & Quiceno, J. (2002). Maternal caregiving and infant security in two cultures. Developmental Psychology, 38, 67-78. Radke-Yarrow, M. (1991). Attachment patterns in children of depressed mothers. In C. M. Parkes, J. Stevenson-Hinde, & P. Marris aids), Attachment Across the Life Cycle (pp.115-126). London: Routledge. Radloff, LS. (1977). The CES-D scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measurement, 1, 385-401 . 160 Renken, B., Egeland, B., Marvinney, D., Mangelsdorf, S., & Sroufe, L. A. (1989). Early childhood antecedents of aggression and passive withdrawal in early elementary school. Journal of Personality, 5 7, 257-281. Ritter, G., & Moyi, P. L. (2003). The impact of day care on school readiness: Using new data to examine the controversy. Paper symposium presented at the Biennial Conference of the Society for Research in Child Development, Tampa, FL. Roberts, RE. (1980). Reliability of the CES-D scale in different ethnic contexts. Psychiatry Research, 2, 125- 1 34. Eisenberg (V 01. Ed), Hancbook of child psychology: Vol. 111. Social, emotional, (Rothbart, M. K., & Bates, J. E. (1998). Temperament. In W. Damon (Series Ed.) & N. and personality development (58- ed., pp. 105-176). New York: Wiley. Rothbaum, F., Pott, M., Azuma, H., Miyake, K., & Weisz, J. (2000). The development of close relationships in Japan and the United States: Paths of symbiotic harmony and generative tension. Child Development, 71, 1121-1142. Rothbaum, F ., Weisz, J ., Pott, M., Miyake, K., & Morelli, G. (2000). Attachment and culture: Security in the United States and Japan. American Psychologist, 55, 1093-1104. Rothbaum, F., Weisz, J ., Pott, M., Miyake, K., & Morelli, G. (2001). Deeper into attachment and culture. American Psychologist, 5 6, 827-829. Rotter, J. B. (1990). Internal versus external control of reinforcement: A case history of a variable. American Psychologist, 45, 489-493. Sagi, A., Lamb, M. E., Lewkowicz, K. S., Shoharn, R., Dvir, R., & Estes, D. (1986). Security of infant-mother, -father, and —metapelet attachments among kibbutz- reared Israeli children. In I. Bretherton & E. Waters (Eds.), Growing points in attachment theory and research. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 50(1-2, Serial No. 209). Scarr, S. (1998). American child care today. American Psychologist, 53, 95-108. Schumacker, R. E., & Lomax, R. G. (1996). A Beginner ’s Guide to Structural Equation Modeling. Lawrence Erlbaum: Mahwah, NJ. Seifer, R., Schiller, M., Samerofl‘, A. J., Resnick, S., & Riordan, K. (1996). Attachment, maternal sensitivity, and infant temperament during the first year of life. Developmental Psychology, 32, 12-25. 161 Shonkoff, J. P. & Phillips, D. A. (2000). From neurons to neighborhoods: the science of early chilcflrood development. Washington, DC: National Academy Press. Sigel, I. E., McGillicuddy-DeLisi, A. V., & Goodnow, J. J. (1992). Parental belief systems: The psychological consequences for children. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum. Smetana, J. G. (1994). Beliefs about parenting: Origins and developmental implications. New Directions for Child Development, 66. Solomon, J. & George, C. (1999). The measurement of attachment security in infancy and childhood. In J. Cassidy & P. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of Attachment: Theory, Research and Clinical Applications (pp. 287-316). New York: Guilford Press. Sofronoff, K., & F arbotko, M. (2002). The effectiveness of parent management training to increase self-efficacy in parents of children with Asperger syndrome. Autism, 6, 271-286. Spieker, S. J ., & Booth, C. L. (1988). Maternal antecedents of attachment quality: What makes social risk risky? In J. Belsky & T. Nezworski (Eds.), Clinical implications of attachment (pp. 95-135). Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum. Spoth, R., & Conroy, S. (1993). Survey of prevention-relevant beliefs and efforts to enhance parenting skills among rural parents. The Journal of Rural Health, 9, 227-239. Sroufe, L. A. (1985). Attachment classification from the perspective of infant-caregiver relationships and infant temperament. Child Development, 5 6, 1-14. Susman-Stillman, A., Kalkoske, M., Egeland, B. & Waldman, I. (1996). Infant temperament and maternal sensitivity as predictors of attachment security. Infant Behavior and Development, 19, 33-47. Teti, D. M., & Gelfand, D. M. (1991). Behavioral competence among mothers of infants in the first year: The mediational role of maternal self-efficacy. Child Development, 62, 918-929. Teti, D. M., Gelfand, D. M., Messinger, D. S., & Isabella, R. (1995). Maternal depression and the quality of early attachment: An examination of infants, preschoolers, and their mothers. Developmental Psychology, 31, 364-3 76. Teti, D. M., Nakagawa, M., Das, R., & Wirth, O. (1991). Security of attachment between preschoolers and their mothers: Relations among social interaction, parenting stress, and mothers’ sorts of the Attachment Q-Set. Developmental Psychology, 27, 440-447. 162 Teti, D. M., O’Connell, M. A., & Reiner, C. D. (1996). Parenting sensitivity, parental depression and child health: The mediational role of parental self-efficacy. Early Development and Parenting, 5, 237-250. Thomas, A., & Chess, S. (1977). Temperament and Development. New York: Brunner/Mazel. Thompson, R A., & Lamb, M. E. (1983). Security of attachment and stranger sociability in infancy. Developmental Psychology, 1 9, 184-191. Tronick, E., Morelli, G. A. & Ivey, P. [C (1992). The infant forager infant and toddler’s patterns of social relationships: Multiple and simultaneous. Developmental Psychology, 28, 568-577. True, M. M., Pisani, L., & Oumar, F. (2001). Infant-mother attachment among the Dogon of Mali. Child Development, 72, 1451-1466. Ullman, J. (1996). Structural equation modeling. In B. Tabachnik & L. Fidel] (Eds.), Using Multivariate Statistics (3rd ed) (pp. 709-811). New York: HarperCollins. van Uzendoom, M. H., & Kroonenberg, P. M. (1988). Cross-cultural patterns of attachment: A meta-analysis of the strange situation. Child Development, 59, 147- 1 56. van IJzendoorn, M. H., & Sagi, A. (1999). Cross-cultural patterns of attachment: Universal and contextual dimensions. In J. Cassidy and P. R. Shaver’s (Eds.) Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications (pp. 713- 734). New York: Guilford Press. Vaughn, B., & Bost, K. K. (1999). Attachment and temperament: Redundant, independent, or interacting influences on interpersonal adaptation and personality development? In J. Cassidy and P. R. Shaver’s (Eds.) Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications. (pp. 713-734). New York: Guilford Press. Vaughn, B., Stevenson-Hinde, J., Waters, E., Kotsafiis, A., Lefever, G. B., Shouldice, A., Trude], M., & Belsky, J. (1992). Attachment security and temperament in infancy and early childhood: Some conceptual clarifications. Developmental Psychology, 28, 463-473. Watamura, S. E., Donzella, B., Alwin, J., & Gunnar, M. R. (2003). Morning-to- afiemoon increases in cortisol concentrations for infants and toddlers at child care: Age differences and behavioral correlates. Child Development, 74, 1006-1020. 163 Waters, E. (1995). Appendix A: The attachment Q-set (version 3.0). In E. Waters, B. Vaughn, G. Posada, & K. Kondo-Ikemura (Eds.), Caregiving, cultural, and cognitive perspectives on secure-base behavior and working models: New growing points of attachment theory and research. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 60 (2-3, Serial No. 244). Waters, E., Wippman, J ., & Sroufe, L. A. (1979). Attachment, positive affect, and competence in the peer group: Two studies in construct validation. Child Development, 50, 821-829. Waters, E. & Deane, K. (1985). Defining and assessing individual differences in attachment relationships: Q-methodology and the organization of behavior in infancy and early childhood. In I. Bretherton & E. Waters (Eds.), Growing points of attachment theory and research. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development. Zahn-Waxler, C., Ianotti, R. J., Cummings, E. M., & Denham, S. (1990). Antecedents of problem behaviors in children of depressed mothers. Development and Psychopathology, 2, 271 -291 . 164 IIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIIII will111111111111W 1293 0250