y “5:. Ya. w a .rrsH . .. :w , r ., 29):...1 .f .2“. t. .93 3...... 2.. 4:. < .6. TEA-lit THESIS 2004 51/9“ L??2 3 ? LIBRARY Michigan State University This is to certify that the dissertation entitled ATTACHMENT, SOCIAL SUPPORT COMPETENCIES, AND VULNERABILITY TO AFFECTIVE SYMPTOMATOLOGY AMONG ADOLESCENT OFFSPRING OF BIPOLAR DISORDER PARENTS presented by Tracy Lynne Smith Simko has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for Ph.D. Counseling Psychology degree in Major professor Date 12/12/03 MS U is an Affirmative Action/Equal Opportunity Institution 0-12771 PLACE IN RETURN BOX to remove this checkout from your record. To AVOID FINES return on or before date due. MAY BE RECALLED with earlier due date if requested. DATE DUE DATE DUE DATE DUE 6/01 c:/ClRC/DateDue.p65-p.15 ATTACHMENT, SOCIAL SUPPORT COMPETENCIES, AND VULNERABILITY TO AFFECTIVE SYMPTOMATOLOGY AMONG ADOLESCENT OFFSPRING OF BIPOLAR DISORDERED PARENTS By Tracy Lynne Smith Simko A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Counseling and Educational Psychology 2003 ABSTRACT ATTACHMENT, SOCIAL SUPPORT COMPETENCIES, AND VULNERABILITY To AFFECTIVE SYMPTOMATOLOGY AMONG ADOLESCENT OFFSPRING OF BIPOLAR DISORDERED PARENTS By Tracy Lynne Smith Simko This study proposed and examined a process by which quality of attachment to parents influences vulnerability for affective symptomatology in offspring of parents with bipolar disorder. A path model was tested wherein three social support competencies - perceived social support, social self-efficacy, and fear of negative evaluation - mediate this relationship. I pursued the idea that secure attachment to parents, in contrast to insecure attachment, guides at-risk offspring to employ constructive assistance-seeking strategies and to evaluate social support in ways that reduce their risk for affective symptomatology. A review of the literature on the process in which secure attachment promotes social support competence that guides the at-n'sk adolescent to successfully access, recognize, and utilize support from important others during times of stress supports this idea. Self-report measures of attachment bonds to father and mother, perceived social support, social self-efficacy, fear of negative evaluation, and affective symptomatology were completed by adolescents who have a parent diagnosed with bipolar disorder. Results did not support the proposed mediational model; however, other relationships among attachment bonds, social support competency measures, and affective symptomatology were demonstrated. Implications for how findings advance current understandings about protection from risk in this population are discussed. Limitations of the study are noted and directions for fiIture research are addressed. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS I would like to acknowledge and wholeheartedly thank those who were instrumental in helping me complete this work. The idea and motivation for this project emerged from my cherished experiences with families of the Rush Presbyterian St. Luke’s Parent Support Group. I am indebted to them for all they taught me about parenting with strength, honesty, humor, and compassion. Dr. Patricia Meaden provided mentorship, enthusiasm, and a realistic perspective that were crucial early in the process. My dear friend, Anne Mauricio, consistently inspired me with her strong work ethic, positive attitude, support, and genuine, empathic understanding. Ellie Derr also deserves thanks for the encouragement she gave me at an important point in the process. My dissertation chair, Dr. Fred Lopez, is responsible for significantly enhancing the quality of this work due to his theoretical expertise, thoroughness and continued commitment to this project. To my family I owe the most. Owen and Connor, lights of my life, always able to lift my spirit so simply; you fill me with a sense of pride that far exceeds any other accomplishment in my life. Greg’s remarkably healthy approach to partnership and parenting always made my working on this project easier on our family; we are blessed by his selflessness, commitment, and affection. Lastly, thanks, Mom, for your lifelong support, the pride that you convey, and the unconditional love that has enabled me to pursue what I value. iii TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES ................................................................................. vii LIST OF FIGURES ............................................................................... viii INTRODUCTION .............................................................................. 1 Bipolar Disorder ........................................................................ 2 Parental Bipolar Disorder and Offspring Vulnerability ........................... 3 Genetics ..................................................................... 3 Parenting deficits associated with bipolar disorder ........................ 4 Impact of parental BPD on offspring ...................................... 5 Aim of the current study ........................................................... 7 LITERATURE REVIEW ....................................................................... 10 Attachment theory: A Framework for Examining Vulnerability to Affective Symptomatology .................................................................. 10 The Theory .................................................................. 10 Internal Working Models .................................................... 13 Attachment Bonds, Parental BPD, and Resilience ....................... 16 Perceived Social Support ............................................................ 20 Fear of Negative Evaluation ......................................................... 23 Social Self Efficacy ................................................................... 24 Path Model Summary ................................................................ 26 SUMMARY OF THE LITERATURE ....................................................... 27 HYPOTHESES ................................................................................ 3O METHODOLOGY .............................................................................. 32 Participants ............................................................................ 32 Parent participants ........................................................... 32 Adolescent participants ..................................................... 33 Procedure .............................................................................. 34 Measures .............................................................................. 37 Demographic Questionnaire ......................................................... 37 Adolescent Psychopathology Scale ....................................... 37 Inventory of Parent and Peer Attachment ................................. 38 Social Provisions Scale ...................................................... 4O Adolescent Social Self Efficacy Scale ..................................... 41 Fear of Negative Evaluation Scale ......................................... 42 Design .................................................................................. 43 Analysis .............................................................................. 43 iv RESULTS 45 oooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooooo Scale Descriptives ................................................................... 45 Preliminary Analyses ............................................................... 47 Demographic variables .................................................... 47 Interrcorrelations Among Demographic Variables and Scales ................. 49 Results of Primary Analyses ....................................................... 51 Relations of adolescents’ attachment bonds with each parent .......... 51 Regression Analysis: Contributions of Parental Attachment Bonds to the Prediction of Adolescent Affective Psychopathology ............................ 51 Regression Analyses: Contributions of Parental Attachment Bonds to the Prediction of Social Competence .................................................. 54 Attachment Bonds and Perceived Social Support ....................... 55 Attachment Bonds and Social Self-Efficacy ............................... 55 Attachment Bonds and Fear of Negative Evaluation .................... 55 Regression Analysis: Contributions of Perceived Social Support, Social Self- Efficacy, and Fear of Negative Evaluation to the Prediction of Adolescent Affective Psychopathology .......................................................... 58 Post Hoc Analyses ...................................................................... 63 DISCUSSION ..................................................................................... 69 Overview .............................................................................. 69 Correlational Analyses ............................................................... 71 Group Comparisons of Secure Parental Attachment ............................... 72 Parental Attachment Bonds Predicting Adolescent Affective Psychopathology. .75 Social Support Competency Predicting Adolescent Affective Psychopathology. 78 Implications for Intervention and Prevention ......................................... 80 Limitations ................................................................................ 82 Recommendations for Future Research ............................................. 84 APPENDICES A. Recruiting Notification .............................................................. 87 B. Reminder Card for Materials Not Received ................................... 88 C. Parent Demographic Questionnaire .............................................. 89 D. Adolescent Demographic Questionnaire ....................................... 91 E. Adolescent Psychopathology Scale ............................................. 93 F. Inventory of Parent and Peer Attachment ...................................... 100 G. Perceived Social Support Scale ................................................. 103 H. Social Self Efficacy Scale ...................................................... 105 1. Fear of Negative Evaluation Scale .............................................. 106 REFERENCES .............................................................................. 107 LIST OF TABLES Table 1: N’s, Means, Standard Deviatons, and Ranges for Key Model Variables Table 2: Results from ANOVA’S and Independent Sammes T Tests Testing Significant Relationships Between Demographic Variables and APS Scores ...... Table 3: Intercorrelations Among Selected Demographic Variables and Key Model Variables .............................................................................. Table 4: Summary of Hierarchical Regression Analysis for Attachment Bonds Predicting APS Scores ............................................................... Table 5: Summary of Hierarchical Regression Analysis for Attachment Bonds Predicting Social Competence Scores ........................................... Table 6: Summary of Hierarchical Regression Analysis for Perceived Social Support Predicting APS Scores .................................................. Table 7: Summary of Hierarchical Regression Analysis for Social Self-Efficacy Predicting APS Scores ................................................... Table 8: Summary of Hierarchical Regression Analysis for Fear of Negative Evaluation Predicting APS Scores .......................................... Table 9: Means, Standard Deviations, and Intercorrelations for the Components of Parental Attachment Bonds (Trust, Communication, and Alienation) and Key Model Variables ............................................. Table 10: Summary of Hierarchical Regression Analysis Trust, Communication, and Alienation Predicting APS Scores ........................... 46 48 53 S9 59 6O 67 Figure 1: Figure 2: Figure 3: Figure 4: LIST OF FIGURES A model that describes the relationship between quality of attachment to parents, PSS, SSE, FNE, and affective symptomatology for adolescents of bipolar parents ..................... Interaction for Regression Analysis: Attachment Bond to Diagnosed Parent & Attachment Bond to Non-Diagnosed Parent as Predictor Variables and APS Scores as Criterion Variable ......... Path Diagram Depicting the Proposed Mediational Model of the Relation Between Parental Attachment Bonds and Adolescent Affective Psychopathology with Standardized Coefficients Interaction Between Attachment Alienation Associated with the Diagnosed Parent and Attachment Alienation Associated with the Non-Diagnosed Parent in Predicting APS Scores ............... viii 29 54 62 69 INTRODUCTION It has been well established that being raised by a parent with an affective disorder places a child at risk for a number of psychiatric and psychosocial problems (see reviews by Beardslee, Versage, & Gladstone, 1998; Downey & Coyne, 1990; Goodman & Gotlib, 1999). In the last two decades, heightened interest in the children of affectively disordered parents has resulted in an extensive empirical literature base documenting particularly higher rates of mood disorders and impaired familial and peer relationships (Cytryn, McKnew, Bartko, Larnour, & Hamovit, 1982; Hammen, Burge, Burney, & Adrian, 1990). Within the large body of literature on risks for children raised by parents with affective disorders there has been an overwhelming focus on children of depressed parents. Relatively few studies examine children who have experienced both parental depression and parental mania despite an alarming prevalence of manic-depression or bipolar disorder among adults and adolescentsl. In a comprehensive study of mortality and disability from diseases, injuries, and risk factors conducted in 1990 and projected to 2020, bipolar disorder was ranked as the sixth leading cause of disability across physical and psychiatric disorders (Murray & Lopez, 1996). When bipolar parents and their offspring are studied, the data are most often collapsed with data from children of depressed parents with far fewer offspring of bipolar disordered parents included. While the disorders often share some symptoms of depression, their etiology, course, and treatment often differ. Nevertheless, a modest but provocative literature on the offspring of bipolar disordered parents exists and indicates that the disorder represents unique ' The terms bipolar disorder and manic depression are used interchangeably in the literature. challenges for offspring. The current study seeks to examine offspring characteristics and coping processes that may render some offspring more or less vulnerable to affective disturbance. Bipolar Disorder Bipolar I disorder (BPD), a mood disorder, is typically a lifelong, episodic condition. The central feature of the disorder is that two kinds of mood episodes of differing polarity —major depressive and manic episodes—reoccur and are interspersed over time with periods of well-being. Bipolar depression is typically manifested by the follOwing symptoms: sleep disturbance, lethargy, inertia and guilt associated with inertia, decreased interest and motivation, a pessimistic view of self, others, and the future, indecisiveness, diminished sense of competence and heightened sense of hopelessness, crying, and social withdrawal. During a depressive episode, oversensitivity to rejection or criticism is common. Persons with bipolar disorder often have difliculty distinguishing between real rejection or criticism and their own cognitive distortions - sometimes able to recognize intellectually that others’ behavior was not intended to be rejecting or critical, but unable to inhibit or reduce their strong emotional reaction (e.g., sadness, guilt, embarrassment, or anger) to that behavior. Social withdrawal is common during depressive episodes. In contrast, the predominant mood of mania may be characterized as elevated, expansive, and irritable, and angry outbursts can occur. Mania is expressed through hyperactivity, flight of ideas, aggressiveness, and inflated self-esteem or grandiosity that often results in a proliferation of new ideas and projects and excessive or obsessive involvement in activities. While manic, individuals can flee unexpectedly (i.e., take spontaneous trips, disappear and reappear without explanation). Sleep disruption is common and can cause a bipolar to feel exhausted but unable to slow down their thoughts or physical energy and drive enough to fall asleep. The lack of sleep can initially lead to more time for adventurous activity. However, unable to maintain the positive benefits of little sleep, irritability and paranoia take the place of euphoria; behavior becomes increasingly disorganized and thinking becomes confused. Problem-solving abilities are substantially diminished during episodes of both depression and mania. Parentaflipolar Disorder and Offspring Vulnegbjliy Genetics. A genetic transmission hypothesis is implicit in most studies of offspring vulnerability to mood disorders. In bipolar disorder, genetics establishes risk. Trait markers of genetic liability are being actively researched but have yet to be identified. So, while direct tests are not yet available, family studies, twin studies, and adoption studies provided an indirect means of testing the genetic hypothesis. For a person who has a parent with BPD, the risk of developing bipolar disorder at some time in his or her life is approximately 12% and the risk of developing recurrent major depressive disorder is also 12% (Rush, Cain, Raese, Stewart, Waller & Debus, 1991; Todd et al., 1996). Thus, there is an approximately 24% chance of a serious recurrent mood disorder of some kind in each offspring of a parent with BPD compared to approximately 1% in non-bipolar families (Lewinshon, Klein, & Seeley, 1995; Weissman, Leaf, & Tischler, 1988). When other affective disorders (e.g., cyclothymia, dysthymia) and subclinical levels of depression are considered, rates increase to as great as 63% (Cytryn et al., 1982; Gershon et al., 1985 ; Grigoroiu-Serbanescu, Christodorescu, Jipescu, Totoescu, Marinescu, & Ardelean, 1989; McKnew, Cytryn, Efron, Gershon, & Bunney, 1979). Davenport, Adland, Gold, & Goodwin (1979) reported that 10 of 36 children in families with BPD in two generations or more developed bipolar disorder. Although the biological transmission of affective illness across generations is supported, Beardslee et al. (1998) noted that even strong proponents of the genetic hypotheses maintain that the environment and its interaction with genetic predisposition must not be overlooked. There is little question that parental depression negatively affects the social-emotional environment within which children develop and that this, in turn, increases a child’s risk for developing unipolar depression (see review by Goodman & Gotlib, 1999). Regarding bipolar disorder, genetic vulnerability is undeniably salient in its etiolog but does not account fully for individual differences in the expression and maintenance of disturbance. For both unipolar depression and bipolar disorder, social and environmental factors are thought to explain the portion of variance not accounted for by genetic transmission and are widely considered to evoke or protect against genetic vulnerability. Parenting deficits associated with BPD. Anthony (1975) was the first to describe impairments in parenting associated with BPD that may place the developing child at risk for depression or other social-emotional problems. Anthony described problems such as: a) taking but refirsing to give, b) having little awareness of others and consequently little ability to empathize with them, and c) having an impoverished capacity to foster stable, secure attachment behaviors and emotional bonds with the child because of the parent’s own dependency needs. Davenport et al. (1979) was the first to describe features of families with bipolar (manic-depressive) parents. These authors described several factors that influence the maintenance of pathology through generations, including the following: avoidance of affect by family members (emotions are threatening), absence of intimate relationships apart from the family, displaced parental low self-esteem, and fears and defensive behavior related to illness heritability. In a follow up study, Davenport et al. (1984) reported that bipolar mothers were more overprotective but less attentive to their children’ 5 health needs and reported more negative affect toward the child. The authors reported that these mothers were disorganized, less active with their children, and more unhappy, tense, and ineffective. Bipolar parents also reported lower scores than controls in the areas of family interaction and social adjustment. Zahn-Waxler et al. (1984) hypothesized that problems in parent-child bonding occur in families where BPD exists because of a lack of parental consistency and stability. Impact of pmenteflPD on offspring, The consequences for children exposed to parental BPD were studied primarily during the 1970’s and 1980’s. Problems of affect regulation and interpersonal relatedness dominate the small but thought-provoking literature. Anthony (1975) argued that children in these conditions suffer inconsistency in their emotional upbringing. He suggested that mood dysregulation may be a likely result if the child becomes enmeshed in the ever-shifting moods of the BPD parent. He also noted that problems of identification with the ill parent can arise, and there may be a tendency for offspring to become “magic helpers” constantly offering assistance to parents to make up for self-felt deficiencies. Mayo, O’Connell, and O’Brien (1979) studied 22 offspring of BPD parents and found that 10 developed separation-anxiety. Twelve nonsymptomatic high-risk children were preoccupied with the health of their families. Cytryn et al. (1982) reviewed clinical and experimental studies of offspring of parents with affective illness, including those with parents with BPD, spanning infancy, childhood, and early adolescence. These authors found a clear tendency toward early disturbances in the areas of affect regulation and social interaction. Gaensbauer et al. (1984) studied social and affective development in infants with a bipolar parent. These infants demonstrated disturbances in the quality of their attachments and problems regulating their emotions adaptively. Zahn—Waxler et al. (1984) also examined the social and emotional functioning of young offspring of manic- depressive parents. They reported difficulties in maintaining friendly social interactions, in sharing, and in helping playmates. The children studied had specific difficulty modulating hostile impulses. The authors suggested that experiencing the ill parent’s emotional extremes in sadness, excitability, and irritability makes such disregulation of affect likely for offspring. Furthermore, the authors noted that the interpersonal and emotional problems of the offspring were similar to those of their parents. These children were followed and reported on four years later; patterns of maladjustment that had been identified earlier were evident at the four-year follow up. The children had more difficulty than controls with insight, empathy, establishing relationships, and regulating affect. Radke-Yarrow et al. (1985) studied the children of 14 BPD mothers and found that they had twice the level of insecure attachment than children of controls. The authors noted consistencies between this study and others that evidence both genetic and environmental influences on offspring development for children of parents with BPD. They, too, attributed offspring difficulties to “contrasting extremes and alterations” in the behavior of the bipolar parent. Taken together, this literature suggests that experiencing the emotional inconsistency and shifting availability of a parent with BPD increases a child’s risk of certain adverse consequences. The children studied had trouble modulating their own mood and had specific problems with insight into their own behavior, sharing, regulating hostility, and demonstrating empathy. These deficiencies were linked to early difficulties establishing positive parent and peer relationships. Aim of the Current Study The focus of the research described above was primarily on young children but the findings have implications for social-emotional development beyond the early childhood years. That is, fiom a developmental perspective, early close relationships frame a child’s subsequent transactions with his or her social world. Childhood difficulties establishing helpful, close bonds with principal caretakers have been linked to adolescent problems with self-esteem and depression, as well as to the use of less adaptive (problem-solving) coping strategies (e. g., seeking outside help in times of need) during adolescence (Armsden & Greenberg, 1987; Greenberg, Seigal, & Leitch, 1983). Such problems are thought to develop in large part because when one has early difficulty establishing close bonds, he or she tends to form sub sequent relationships that are not supportive and are easily disrupted (Carlson & Sroufe, 1995). Unfortunately, empirical research investigating the adolescent functioning of those young children involved in the initial offspring studies described above has not been reported. Furthermore, attempts to understand the impact of parental BPD on adolescent psychological functioning have not progressed beyond documenting rates of offspring psychopathologyz. Although provocative, studies that simply correlate parental BPD with adolescent disturbance leave some important questions unanswered. For example, what enables some at-risk adolescents to fare better than others? Many children who grow up with mentally ill parents, including those with parents who have BPD, adapt successfirlly despite significant adversity. Furthermore, are there certain person- characteristics that serve to buffer individuals from the negative consequences of parental BPD? The answers to these questions are undoubtedly complex and multifaceted. Nevertheless, research that directly addresses risk and protective factors for adolescent offspring is needed so that interventions to reduce psychopathology can be developed. Even in the extensive literature base on children of depressed mothers, explanations of why some children are able to withstand significant parental emotional disturbance without developing symptoms themselves remain understudied (Goodman & Gotlib, 1999). prrogress in treating and preventing adolescent mood disturbance related to parental BPD is to be made, a move to more sophisticated, process-oriented explanations of resilience is necessary. One important next step is to explore individual differences that may reduce vulnerability. The current study represents such a step. The present study used a process-oriented conceptual model - John Bowlby’s attachment theory (1969, 1980, 1988) - to investigate a set of characteristics and associated coping strategies that may render some adolescent offspring more resilient than others. Attachment researchers have demonstrated that for adolescents, parental attachment relationships explain a moderate amount of variability in outcome measures of emotional adjustment (Armsden & Greenberg, 1987; Greenberg, Siegel, & Leitch, 1984). Despite a growing consensus that parental attachment plays a significant role in reducing vulnerability to affective psychopathology, virtually no empirical data clarifying the explanatory mechanisms underlying this relationship are available. To address this 2 Mood disorders are the most prevalent form of psychopathology for this population. limitation, the current study investigates a path model for understanding offspring resilience using a sample of adolescents who have experienced parental bipolar disorder. LITERATURE REVIEW Attachment Theory: A Framework for Examining Vulnerability to Affective Smmcrmfalcgy Attachment theory has been used as a framework for explaining adaptational risks for children exposed to parental illness, in particular risks for affective disturbance (Bowlby, 1969, 1973, 1980; Cummings & Cicchetti, 1990). In the past decade the theory “has gained increasing theoretical and research attention for its heuristic value in explaining how parental closeness can serve as a protective buffer and source of security throughout adolescence” (Kenny, Moilanen, Lomax, & Brabeck, 1993, p. 409). Before elaborating on links between attachment bonds and affective symptomatology, the relationship between attachment and social-emotional development is reviewed. The Theog Attachment is generally defined as an important enduring emotional bond of substantial intensity. Bowlby’s (1969) attachment theory emphasizes bonds that develop between an infant and one or more caregivers as the first manifestation of the attachment system — a behavioral system that serves to protect the child and promote survival. Recently the theory has been used to guide research that describes emotional and psychological disturbances that can result at any age from the actual or threatened disruption of attachment bonds. The essence of the theory is that human beings exhibit greater social and emotional adjustment when they have confidence in the accessibility and responsiveness 10 of a trusted other or attachment figure’ . Two major dimensions of attachment have been suggested (Parkes & Stevenson-Hinde, 1982): a) the cognitive-affective dimension, defined as the underlying quality of affect toward attachment figures, and b) the behavioral dimension, defined as the utilization of these figures for support in times of stress and need. The core assumption of attachment theory is that maintaining relatedness to others is a universal and fundamental survival need. Rooted in infancy, the attachment system is activated under conditions of “felt distress”; caregiver responses under these conditions determine not only the infants’ physical safety but also his or her inner sense of security. AS the child develops, organized patterns of behavior emerge to regulate proximity to attachment figures and maintain attachment bonds. As cognitive capacities increase, experiences with caregivers are organized into “internal working models” (IWMs) of the self and the attachment figure. These models guide expectations of self and others in relationships and contribute to one’s developing sense of relational competence. Consequently, IWM’S influence one’s selection of coping strategies for dealing with stress, family stress in particular". Mary Main (1990) described the attachment system as “context sensitive”; that is, different organizations and types of behavior are required to maintain proximity to caregivers depending on the anticipated availability of the caregiver. Sensitive, consistent patterns of caregiver responsiveness early in life tend to produce securely attached infants. With a foundation of a “secure base” in others, securely attached 3 Attachment figures are most often parents, therefore the current study focuses primarily on parent- adolescent attachment relationships and secondarily on peer-adolescent attachment relationships. 11 children move out into the world, pursue new experiences and relationships, and feel confident they can return to find attachment figures supportive if they are needed (Bowlby, 1988). With secure attachment as a base, children spend more and more time exploring the environment and tolerating separation from caregivers with less distress (Ainsworth, 1989). When they do feel threatened, securely attached children communicate with attachment figures in ways that promote responsiveness (Cassidy, 1994). In turn, fears and stress are sufficiently alleviated so that exploration of the environment continues. Repeated positive experiences of seeking and receiving assistance from attachment figures when distressed instills confidence or “felt security” in the accessibility of one or more trustworthy others who can be relied upon to help regulate future distress. Over time, this confidence becomes internalized - securely attached children incorporate lessons learned fiom the reliable, sensitive caregiving of attachment figures and develop the capacity to effectively regulate their own emotions. As they become increasingly skilled socially, children learn to balance interpersonal dependence and independence. Having used their primary caregiver as a “secure base,” securely attached infants demonstrate more enthusiasm, positive affect, and persistence in problem-solving situations as toddlers (Matas, Arend, & Sroufe, 197 8) and respond more flexibly, persistently, and resourcefully in preschool (Sroufe, 1983). During middle childhood and adolescence, individuals with secure attachment histories show less negative reactions to periods of high family stress (Pianta, Egeland, & Sroufe, 1990; Sroufe, Levy, & Carlson,1998). " Stress results from an appraisal that the demands of a situation exwed one’s resources. Here, it specifically refers to threats to the survival of the attachment bond, and, ultimately, to the survival of the individual. 12 In contrast, an insecure attachment orientation develops when caregivers are unresponsive, inconsistent, interfering, or when they insufficiently or inaccurately read and respond to the child’s signals. In these environments, the child is more likely to develop a sense of self as unworthy of care and/or a model of important others as uncaring or undependable. Emotional reactions to insensitive caregiving can be intense (e. g., anger, rage, despair). Behavioral reactions range from detachment to hypervigilence toward the signals of the attachment figure, depending upon the child’s subjective experience of others. On one hand, when children experience attachment figures as consistently inaccessible, motivation to continue help seeking diminishes. Warm behaviors that promote emotional intimacy fade over time. As hopelessness and emotional detachment increase the likelihood that the child will effectively communicate future needs to helpful others diminishes. On the other hand, if children experience caregivers as unpredictable, the child may become hypervigilant to the attachment behaviors (or lack there of) of important others to capture whatever attention may be available. Despite a strong desire for close relationships, these individuals are ambivalent about the trustworthiness of important others. A tendency to worry excessively about being evaluated and rejected makes it more difficult for the insecurely attached to establish healthy, supportive relationships. Interestingly, the tendency to inhibit or exaggerate emotions (Izard, Haynes, Chisholm, & Baak, 1991) may mirror the affective system of the attachment figure. Intml working models. Although attachment is initially behavioral (i.e., parent- child interaction), models of self and other develop attachment experiences gradually become internalized cognitively by the child. Though models are progressively l3 constructed and are modifiable in response to significant changes in circumstances, mental models grow increasingly firm over time as our ability to seek, shape, and interpret experience converges with a tendency to view situations in ways that conform with prior experience. IWMs may therefore be conceptualized as “interpersonal schemas” (Safran, 1990) that guide how individuals interpret stressfirl interpersonal events. As a young child experiences repeated relational success via coordinated emotional communication with attachment figures (e.g., “If I show Mom that I am frightened, she will help me feel less so”) working models of self and important others gradually begin to serve as a lens through which one views the relational behavior of important others. This lens focuses attention, filters information, and guides responses to stress, often operating at an unconscious level (Bowlby, 1988). Accordingly, as the young child develops, he or she becomes a more active force in the parent-child relationship. He or she begins choosing which relationships to seek out and the manner in which he or She participates in these, depending upon the degree to which they meet emotional needs. Children with negative models of self and/or others spend valuable time focused on their interaction with attachment figures to reduce distress. This distraction impedes opportunities to build relational skills. Dysfunctional personal schema are, consequently, more likely. For example, repeated unsatisfying experiences with caretakers can lead to the self-view that one is not capable of establishing firlfilling relationships (Bowlby, 1980). Despite longing for helpful, trusting interpersonal connections, the insecurely attached child anticipates and becomes hypersensitive to inconsistency and rejection. This makes him or her more likely to interpret subsequent interpersonal stress as threatening and respond accordingly. Unfortunately, these children 14 not only expect rejection from others but actually are often rejected by them (Sroufe, 1983) Conversely, securely attached infants are likely to be less aggressive, and more cooperative, sympathetic, and competent in play when they are older (Ainsworth, 1983). Securely attached school—age children are more competent in their relationships with peers and adults, less fearfirl of strangers, and less prone to behavior problems (e. g., social withdrawal and anxiety) and dependent upon adults (Erickson, Sroufe, Egeland, & Byron, 1985; Sroufe, 1983). During adolescence, while attachment behavior is often directed toward non-parental figures, the ability to develop new supportive relationships continues to be significantly influenced by beliefs about one’s relational competence brought forward from early parent-child relationships (Weiss, 1982). Although adolescence may be marked by periods during which parent accessibility is not necessary for adolescent “felt security,” confidence in parental commitment remains crucial. During this time the actual presence of the attachment figure may become less important than the perceived adequacy of attachment relationships. In a study of late adolescence, internal representations of the quality of early childhood attachments significantly related to affect regulation and representations of self and others (Kobak & Sceery, 1988). Securely attached adolescents are rated by peers as being more ego-resilient, less anxious, and less hostile than less securely attached adolescents. Hence, by adolescence, secure attachment can be conceptualized as a constructive interpersonal schema characterized by: a) a positive, coherent view of self that allows the adolescent to encapsulate stress and prevent it from spreading to the entire self-structure; b) an optimistic attitude toward others and problems such that stressfirl interpersonal 15 events are appraised in benign terms, enabling more constructive coping strategies to be employed; and, c) cognitive flexibility which permits one to revise schemas with new information and adapt to environmental change. Such a positive cognitive schema makes depression less likely according to cognitive models of depression (Beck, 1967). Empirical research indeed demonstrates that secure attachment during adolescence reduces risk for depression and other forms of psychopathology in general adolescent populations (Armsden, McCauley, Greenbery, Burke, & Mitchell, 1990; Kobak, Sudler, & Gamble, 1991) and, more specifically, for adolescents exposed to parental depression (Downey & Coyne, 1990). Attachment Bonds Parental BPD. and Resilience Imagine the experience of a child whose bipolar-disordered parent fluctuates between periods of over-involvement to times of marked withdrawal from them. During a manic episode, the parent may become significantly more dramatic and grandiose about his or her self-presentation, obsessive about normally inconsequential matters, and significantly more intrusive and controlling of others in his or her immediate social space. Mania may then give way to depression, wherein the parent becomes self-absorbed, disinterested in the activity or behavior of the child, and overwhelmed by feelings of insecurity and beliefs that they are incapable of helping the child. Under these circumstances, how the child construes the situation and copes with this relationship instability should significantly impact his or her own vulnerability to affective disturbance Attachment security acts as an inner resource to facilitate successful coping. A child with a secure attachment history is more likely to deal with distress by acknowledging it and then will appraise stress, and the efforts required to reduce it, as 16 manageable. He or she is more likely to enact active, constructive coping strategies, such as turning to others for support. Because the securely attached child carries an underlying sense of trust in self and others he or she is less likely to engage in self-blame or become hostile towards attachment figures. He or she is also more likely to view the self as resourcefirl and others as helpful. In addition, individuals with secure attachments have a higher tolerance for unpredictability, disorder, and ambiguity. This would be particularly beneficial for children exposed to parental BPD — cognitive flexibility should render one less distressed by unpredictable emotional episodes than individuals with more rigid cognitive schemata. For instance, for those who experience dramatic parental mood shifts, rules for coping behavior that hold in one affective state don’t necessarily hold in the other (Pound, 1991). In this way, a secure attachment history would enable some offspring to endure parental mood swings and to be flexible in responding to changing circumstances. In a study of psychiatrically hospitalized adolescents, including those with affective disorders, Rosenstein and Horowitz (1996) indeed found that attachment organization was “overwhelmingly insecure.” These authors suggested that adolescent symptomatology was related to a tendency to focus on the ill parent, inhibited exploratory behavior, and the subsequent failure to develop effective affect regulation strategies. In contrast, the insecurely-attached child, more sensitive to both rejection and inconsistency and less tolerant of negative affect, is more likely to feel threatened by such parental behavior. More specifically, attachment theory holds that the child who has been unable to establish a sense of attachment security would react to such relational disappointment in one of two general ways -- by either becoming preoccupied with his or 17 her own feelings or by fiercely turning away from them. In the first case, he or she might ruminate on the negative thoughts and feelings generated by the parent’s behavior and focus on memories of similar experiences. Plagued by a sense of helplessness and alienation, distress may be overwhelming and may spread to other areas of life (e. g., school performance, peer relationships). Intense emotional distress may continue even after the actual threat has terminated (e.g., during a period in which the BPD parent is functioning well). This child is more likely to cope by intensifying efforts to get the BPD parent to comfort him or her during periods of the illness when the parent is incapable of doing so rather than accessing more usefirl support-providers (e.g., the undiagnosed parent). In the second case, the insecurely attached child might exhibit a more defensive style, emphasizing the untrustworthiness of significant others. This child would likely prioritize maintaining distance from potentially helpfirl others and would minimize overt expressions of distress; both of which make it less likely that needed support is obtained. Furthermore, such defensiveness is typically accompanied by an underlying sense of anxiety. In the long run, this makes it more difficult for the defensive (insecurely attached) child to mitigate pain associated with parental affective episodes. However, the negative effect of such variable parental emotionality would conceivably be less if other attachment figures are available or when the child’s environment otherwise provides a sense of stability. Offspring are more likely to develop a sense of trust in others and, in turn, a sense of self as resourceful if structures and resources in the home are efl’ective in: a) alleviating stress for family members and the ill parent; and, b) for providing stability for the children. Given such circumstances, the risk for developing affective psychopathology should be less than when the environment is 18 compromised by parental psychopathology. Secure attachment enables one to realistically evaluate attachment-related experiences and develop relational competence so that during periods of parental unavailability, alternative sources of support are sought and utilized to alleviate distress. In contrast, when instability is prominent and other attachment figures are unavailable, offspring vulnerability to psychopathology should be highest because there are fewer opportunities to develop relational competence (e. g., accurately reading behaviors, particularly emotional, of others) that might buffer them from firture stress. Insecure attachment strategies may seem adaptive when sensitive caregiving is compromised; however, the long-terrn consequences (e. g., inability to develop close, positive relationships in the future) are a diminished sense of personal resourcefirlness/control that would likely exacerbate risk for affective psychopathology. A specific example demonstrates how secure attachment, in contrast to insecure attachment, might protect a child from the negative effects of parental BPD. Envision a manic parent over-reacting to an expression of sadness from a child who misses a friend that has recemly moved to another town. The manic parent might dramatize the sadness, ridicule the child to others, propose unreasonable solutions for seeing the missed child, and/or become irritable, dismissing the child’s feelings. Alternatively, during an episode of depression the BPD parent might respond by intense crying, verbally obsess about additional losses in their own or the child’s life, or express feelings of helplessness and withdraw. Ifthe child has been able to achieve a sense of security within their attachment relationships, he or she is equipped with a sense of social competence and corresponding tendency to effectively access, recognize, and utilize social support; this renders him or 19 her less likely to be disappointed by the parent’s reaction and less likely to internalize the response as rejection. Although the adolescent children of BPD parents have not been empirically studied, young offspring have been. These studies (described earlier in this review) demonstrate that secure attachment indeed buffers young offspring from developing affective symptomatology. These studies, and those in the more general adolescent literature suggest that adolescent offspring of BPD parents are at greater risk for affective disturbance if they have a history of insecure attachment relationships. However, the correlational nature of the existing data do little to explain the relationship between attachment and affective disturbance for at-risk children. Empirical identification of the mechanisms linking attachment and affective symptomatology in this population would strengthen the theoretical model of attachment and depression, would increase understandings of parental BPD and offspring vulnerability to affective symptoms, (Cummings & Cicchetti, 1990) and may extend to other models linking parental behavior and offspring resilience. For the current investigation, a comprehensive literature review revealed three potential mediators of the relationship between attachment and affective symptomatology: l) perceived social support, 2) social self-efficacy, and 3) fear of negative evaluation. Additionally, there is support for examining the moderating role that the non-diagnosed parent may play in reducing vulnerability. Perceived Social Support Perceived social support (PSS) refers to a generalized appraisal that individuals develop in which they believe that they are cared for and valued, that significant others 20 are available to them in times of need, and that they are satisfied with the relationships they have (Procidano & Heller, 1983). Perceptions are initially developed through early close relationship experiences (Sarason, Pierce, & Sarason, 1990); parental attitudes towards a child and the behavior associated with them gradually become incorporated by the child into generalized expectations about the supportiveness of others and one’s own support worthiness. As such, PSS tends to be relatively consistent across time even when individual social circumstances change (Sarason et al., 1986; Sarason, Sarason, & Shearin, 1986). Children whose parental relationships have been supportive and well matched to the child’s needs develop optimistic expectations about the willingness of others to provide support. In contrast, children whose needs for support are not met in such a way develop a general view of others as unwilling or unable of meeting their needs for support (Coble, Gantt, & Mallinckrodt, 1996). In this way parental support influences cognitive schemata regarding the self (e.g., I am worthy of support and valued by others) and the availability of supportive relationships (e.g., that others wish to be of help). Persons are more likely to develop confidence in their own abilities to manage relational distress if they believe that social support exists. In an empirical study of the relationship between adolescent PSS and models of self and other, Blain, Thompson, and Whiffen (1993) demonstrated that secure attachment was associated with high levels of perceived social support from family and fiiends. PSS is also associated with vulnerability to affective disturbance. Research demonstrates that an inverse relationship exists between PSS and depression and between PSS and manic-depression; higher levels of support correlate with lower levels of symptomatology (O’Connell, Mayo, Eng, Jones, Gabel, 1985; Stefos, Bauwens, Staner, 21 Pardoen, & Mendlewics, 1996; Johnson, Winett, Beyer, Greenhouse, & Miller, 1999). Moreover, while the link between PSS and vulnerability to affective symptomatology is not clearly understood, one explanation may be that perceptions of social support influence physiological processes (e.g., cardiovascular functioning, immune functioning) (Uchino, Cacioppo, & Kiecolt-Glaser, 1996); this is particularly intriguing given the biological foundation of bipolar disorder. For children of affectively ill parents, Beardslee and Podorefsky (1988) maintained that interpersonal relationships can protect offspring from developing their own symptoms. A deep commitment to relationships in particular was linked to resiliency. One study specifically exarrrined the role of social support in outcomes for children raised by a parent with BPD. Pellegrini et al. (1986) compared the personal and social resources in children of patients with BPD to those of controls. They found that psychiatric well-being is related to the quality of personal and social resources one has; nondisordered probands demonstrated a strikingly superior profile of personal resources in comparison to disordered probands. Caution is warranted in generalizing from one study, and the retrospective nature of this research prohibits causal interpretations. Nevertheless, the authors’ arguments for additional research investigating links between PSS and vulnerability to affective disorders for at-risk adolescents are compelling. Several studies indicate that a positive relationship with the non-ill parent in particular may buffer the offspring from the negative impact of the ill-parent’s affective disturbance. Rutter (1987) specifically emphasized the protective role of the non- disturbed parent in providing a source of support and consistency for offspring of afl'ectively ill parents. This is especially relevant for the current study, as parental BPD is 22 characterized by mood swings that are typically inconsistent and unpredictable. Similarly, Fisher, Kokes, Cole, Perkins, and Wynne (1987) reviewed the literature on competent children at risk and found that perceptions of support from the non-ill parent characterized well-functioning children of disturbed parents. To summarize the link between attachment and PSS, confidence in the availability of others during times of need, coupled with the ability to accurately read the signals of others should lead to higher levels of PSS for the securely attached. Children with insecure attachment histories usually have lost confidence that important others are accessible and willing to offer support in times of need (Bowlby, 1973). Furthermore, when those with insecure attachment histories do feel threatened and experience associated negative emotions, they communicate this in ways that are less likely to promote responsiveness fi‘om others (Cassidy & Kobak, 1988; Kobak & Sceery, 1988). They also tend to direct heightened distress-related emotions (e.g., desperation, anger, hostility) toward attachment figures, thereby further diminishing the likelihood that helpful others will be available when needed. This cycle should increase vulnerability to affective disturbance as it inhibits environmental exploration and reduces self-confidence in building new potentially supportive relationships (Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters, & Wall, 1978) Mf Negative Evm Included in the notion that working models guide beliefs and expectations about social relationships, one’s belief about how they are perceived by others is correlated with the model of self (Sarason, Peirce, Shearin, Sarason, Waltz, & Poppe, 1990) and theoretically linked with parental attachment and risk for affective disturbance. That is, 23 securely attached individuals value close relationships and hold optimistic beliefs about how others perceive them. They are less Sensitive to rejection than those who are insecurely attached and more likely to have emotional needs met through stable, trusting relationships. Because their own views of self and appraisals of how other’s view them are more positive, objective, and flexible, persons with secure attachment histories are better equipped to manage relational stress and environmental change (Rosenstein & Horowitz, 1996). Thus, the higher an adolescents’ sense of secure parental attachment, the less preoccupied or debilitated he or she should be by the relational stress and environmental change that so often characterizes families with a psychologically- distressed parent. The often unpredictable parental mood swings and erratic behavior of a bipolar parent make it more likely that offspring will be subjected to negativity and family stress within the home (Hodgins, Faucher, Zarac, & Ellenbogen, 2002) and shame and stigma outside the home (Lefley, 1989; Oestman & Kjellin, 2002). As high anxiety about others’ negative evaluations increases risk for depression (O’Connor, Berry, Weiss, and Gilbert, 2002) the adolescent who has been able to develop a strong sense of parental attachment and thus a stronger sense of self will have less apprehension about others’ evaluations. This, in turn, should serve to reduce his or her own vulnerability to affective psychopathology. Social Self-Efficacy (SSE) One’s belief in his or her ability to effect social interaction, or social self-efiicacy (SSE), is also linked theoretically and empirically to social competence, attachment and risk for affective disturbance. It has been suggested that deficits (real or imagined) in the 24 ability to affect others’ emotional states contributes to a diminished sense of social competence, namely social anxiety (V ertue, 2003). However, attachment theorists assert that a secure relationship with one or both parents throughout childhood contributes to the development of a positive schemata about one’s own social-effectiveness (SSE) (Mallinckrodt, 1992). Coordinated communication and a history of relational successes are incorporated by the child into beliefs about his or her sense of socially competency. The more optimistic one is about his or her ability to impact the outcome of stressful events, the less vulnerable one is to using dysfunctional cognitive processes and, in turn, to developing depression (Beck, 1967). Similarly, Dozier, Stoval and Albus (1999) noted that depression may be linked to a “sense of uncontrollability [for stressful events] on the part of the child (p. 500).” This is particularly relevant for offspring of BPD parents, where intermittent and largely unpredictable mood swings characterize the environment. Among early to middle adolescents, parent attachment was found to be associated with lesser hopelessness and a less extemally-oriented locus of control and with greater self- management (coping) skills (Armsden et al., 1987). Cognitive models of depression would benefit from research that elucidates the environmental and familial precursors of the dysfirnctional cognitive processes described in current models of depression (e. g., Beck) (Coble, Gantt, & Mallinckrodt, 1996). In one empirical study, Ehrenberg and Cox (1991) found an inverse relationship between social self-efficacy beliefs and depression; however, studies that investigate the underlying mechanism between SSE and psychopathology are rare. Taken together, PSS, SSE, and FNE are conceptualized in the current study as social support competencies that guide individuals to organize his or her social world in 25 such a way that risk for affective symptomatology is either increased or reduced. “Competency” refers to both a set of interpersonal relationship skills and experiences and the beliefs [about the relational self and others] that prompt an individual to employ those skills to recruit social support. Path Model Summary As described in this review, the attachment system is organized around maintaining proximity to caretakers and, later, to those emotionally close to us. As we develop, affectional bonds that have been established with caregivers widen and change as we enter into other relationships. By adolescence, secure attachment is typified by emotional closeness with several others and trust in their availability and responsiveness. Securely attached adolescents are able to display positive emotions that enhance social interaction and social competence. They are also able to tolerate negative affect while remaining engaged with others (Sroufe, Schork, Frosso, Keroski, & LaFrenier, 1985). It is the argument of the current study that social support competence (high levels of PSS and SSE and low levels of FNE) leads one to engage socially in a manner that should render some at-risk adolescents more resilient to the negative impact of their parent’s bipolar illness than those who are less socially competent. PSS, SSE, and FNE are conceptualized here as related but unique elements of social support competence. Each competency is included in the path model under investigation because of its potential to explain variation in the relationship between parental attachment bonds and offspring affective symptomatology. In sum, one’ s motivation to seek healthy relationships and success in doing so is influenced by these aspects of social support competence that develop from repeated satisfying relationship experiences. The more an individual 26 perceives that support is available when needed and the more he or she believes they can effect interpersonal interaction, the more likely they are to successfully access and utilize support from others when distressed (e.g., during periods of extreme parental mood fluctuation). Similarly, the more optimistic persons are about their own social status, the less vulnerable they should be to developing anxiety or depression when faced with relational stress. Summary of the Literature For offspring of parents with bipolar disorder, the risk for developing affective disturbance is high. Although genetics establishes risk, psychosocial factors impact whether that risk is heightened or attenuated. However, research that addresses just how psychosocial factors influence risk is rare. The development and evaluation of mediational models that demonstrate how these variables exert their influence are needed so that more effective preventative and therapeutic interventions can be developed. Hypothetical links among the variables should be tested, with the most promising models subjected to further experimentation and analysis. In the current study, theoretical and empirical evidence is presented that supports an investigation of the relationship between quality of parental attachment bonds, aspects of social support competence, and offspring vulnerability for affective symptomatology. This support comes fi'om three main sources: the broad developmental psychopathology literature, an extensive literature on offspring of depressed parents, and the small but persuasive literature on children of BPD parents. Across these bodies of literature, secure attachment, in contrast to insecure attachment, is associated with better outcomes for offspring of parents with affective disturbance. Extant theoretical and empirical research 27 suggests that parental attachment quality affects the development and deployment of various social support competencies that may function as the means by which affective symptoms are either inhibited or amplified. Rice, Cunningham, and Young (1997) found that the association of parental attachment bonds to emotional adjustment was indeed mediated by social competence in a large sample (N= 630) of Black and White adolescents. Under the stressful conditions associated with having a BPD parent, attachment organization presumably guides the adolescent’s expectations of availability of social support (perceived social support) and of their own resourcefulness in accessing and utilizing such support (social self—efficacy). Additionally, secure attachment decreases pessimism and anxiety about how others view the self (fear of negative evaluation). Therefore, in cases where a parent with BPD has been able to call upon supportive others or supportive others have otherwise been available to meet his or her needs for safety and comfort during affective episodes, the child is more likely to develop a sense of others as trustworthy and a sense of self as worthy of care. Trust that attachment figures understand and respect one’s needs and that they can be relied upon for help should lead the individual to feel more confident in their own relational capability. This, in turn, renders the individual more likely to seek others under future conditions of stress and to accurately read signs of support from others. In this way, attachment orientation guides the adolescent to actively turn to or avoid others during stressful times. In contrast, adolescents with insecure attachments fear others’ evaluations and are less likely to effectively seek or establish support from needed others during times of stress. These individuals are more likely to exhibit anger toward or emotional detachment from attachment figures when attachment bonds are threatened. This, in 28 combination with their more negativistic attitudes and compromised ability to recognize support when it is available renders them more susceptible to affective symptomatology. Figure 1 demonstrates the relationship between quality of attachment to parents, social support competence, and affective symptomatology. Figure l. A model that describes the relationship between quality of attachment toJarents, PSS, SSE, FNE, and affective symptomatology for adolescents of bipolar parents. Attachment Bond to BPD $ Parent V Affective / \ —> Sympto- matology Trust Communi- cation Alienation Trust \ ) Attachment / t 1 Bond to Communi- L—y cation Non-BPD @ T D Parent : .05. However, a two-way analysis of variance showed that gender of the diagnosed parent was significantly related to APS scores (F (4,85) = 12.11, p < .001). Adolescents who had a diagnosed father had higher APS scores (M= 2.47, SD = .24) than those adolescents with a diagnosed mother (M = 2.193, SD = .41). The interaction term (gender of the adolescent * gender of the parent) was not significant (F (1,93) = 1.443, p > .05. Thus, when the adolescent’s father is the parent who has been diagnosed with bipolar disorder, adolescent psychopathology scores are statistically higher regardless of the gender of the child. One-way analysis of variance showed that group differences exist regarding level of parent education, F (2,89) = 5.72, p < .01. Tukey’s post hoc comparisons showed that adolescents whose parents had attended college (M = 2.30) or graduate training (M = 2.39), scored higher on the APS than those whose parents had completed high school only (M = 1.88). This comparison was significant at the p < .05 level. 47 One-way analysis of variance showed group differences on APS scores between five or fewer sessions. adolescents who reported that they had received different amounts of individual cormseling, F (3,84) = 3.381, p < .05. Adolescents who have received 16 or more individual counseling sessions scored lower on the APS than those who have received Findings from an independent samples t-test procedure showed that adolescent APS than those who had not. Table 2 Reaults from ANOVAsJand Indgpendent Samples T Tests Testing Sigm'ficant psychotropic medication use was significantly associated with APS scores, t (96) = -2.862, p < .01. Adolescents who had taken psychotropic medication scored lower on the Relationships Between Demoggphic Variables and APS Scores“ Demographic H M 8_D Variable Gender of the Diagnosed Parent Male 36 2.47 .24 Female 60 2.20 .41 Parent Education High School 6 1.88 .45 College 57 2.30 .35 College + 29 2.40 .29 # Adolescent Individual Therapy Sessions 47 2.35 .34 1-5 17 2.36 .42 6-15 10 2.26 .23 16 + 14 2.01 .46 Adolescent Psychotropic Medication use Yes 15 2.03 .26 No 73 2.34 .38 48 Note. Only demographic variables reaching statistical significance are presented. lnterrcorrelations AmonaDemographic Variables and Scales. A correlation matrix demonstrating relationships among the demographic variables that were associated with this study’s outcome variable and the key model variables is presented in Table 3. It was expected that indexes of attachment bonds to parents would be significantly and positively correlated with each measure of social support (after fear of negative evaluation scores were reversed) and with the outcome measure. Only partial support for these relationships was found. Only indexes of attachment bonds to the diagnosed parent were significantly correlated to measures of social support competence, namely perceived social support and social self-efficacy. Unexpectedly, parental attachment bonds were not significantly correlated with APS scores. As expected, all measures of social support competence were significantly and positively correlated with each other. Of all the predictor variables studied, only fear of negative evaluation was correlated with APS scores. 49 _o. .v as.» dmo. V Q a com: 33 5:862: 6: cc 5523 2365 3.58 2: :2: seem emcee Etc-:6 £3 8: 222362: 39:55me 508225 .wEBmcsoo use 5:325 .3 296. $sz 9.28:2: 35% 5:3: :23 Joint? 325ch 803 .3233 wczcmcaoo 3:239: .8 398:: one 5:83» :8ch .xowEmEoEExm :wE 3:365 38% :wE E; .8732; 3035250 325852 3.58 mm< Swo—ofimaofixma egos-ca, Euomc_oc< u mad. ”cones—gm 02$ch Lo Lao-a umZu .zumoc-Lm-tcm Eoom u mmm ”toaasm Euom c3680“. u mmm clog-Iona -- E25. nomocwaa .Lo 3980 .o_ 2.- -- 33 :28232 uaobozoxmmcccomflocac .o 5.- sum. -- mcofimcm wE—cmcsou .3636:— Co cons—:2 .w Lem.- rremr 2.- l. 22.833. 23:23 <- remmr E .- .1.er rem-o... ma< .o 3.- tom. mo. :2. 2mm. -- mZ-L .m- ioe. oo. 5. 1.5m.- _o. .13. -- mmm .v o_. crwmr imr rmmr rmm. rewm. Iwm. -- mmn. .m Eccmq ucmocch-co: tam.- N- _ .- 3.- 5. oo. o _ .- w _ .- mo. 2 econ EcEsuSE .m ESE ocmocwflc o. _o.- _o. .m. rmmr vor 3.- .13. tom. 2. -- econ Ecfizomc< ._ 2 a w e c m e m N _ smelt; 833:; E52 GM Em 333:; DEG-EmoEcQ cocoa—cm wcoE< macaw—ctoagfi m 2ch 50 Results of Primg Analyses Relations of adolescents' attachment bonds with each parent. To test the first hypothesis that in comparison to the relationship between the adolescent and the BPD parent, the relationship between the adolescent and the non-diagnosed parent should reflect a higher degree of secure parent-adolescent attachment, a paired-sample t test was conducted and sample means compared. Results showed that the hypothesis was not supported. The mean score for the index of attachment bond between the adolescent and his or her diagnosed parent was 90.96 (SD = 16.58), whereas the mean score for his or her non-diagnosed parent was 85.17 (SD = 21.65), t(96) = 2.262, p = .026. Adolescents in this sample reported more secure attachment bonds with the BPD parent than with the non-diagnosed parent. Ragression Aaalysis: Contributions of Parental Attachment Bonds to the Prediction of Adolescent Affective Psychopathology A hierarchical regression was conducted to test the hypothesis that after controlling for selected demographic variables, indexes of attachment bonds to the diagnosed parent and to the non-diagnosed parent would each make independent and significant contributions to the prediction of adolescent affective symptomatology. In addition, the hierarchical regression tested the hypothesis that, after controlling for main effects, the interaction of indexes of attachment bonds with each parent would make unique, significant contributions to the prediction of APS scores. The demographic variables: Gender of the diagnosed parent, parent education, number of adolescent individual counseling sessions, and adolescent psychotropic medication use were entered as covariates in step one. At step two, indexes of attachment bond to the diagnosed 51 parent and attachment bond to the non-diagnosed parent were regressed on the measure of adolescent affective psychopathology. At step three, the interaction effect of these respective bonds was tested. As recommended by Aiken and West (1991), measures of attachment were centered prior to creating the interaction terms and testing their significance to reduce multicolinearity effects. Results are presented in Table 4. The covariate block entered at step one was a significant predictor of APS scores; the covariates collectively accounted for 23% of the variance in APS scores (132 = .232, p < .001). Unexpectedly, the addition of parental attachment bonds at the second step was not significant in adding to the prediction of APS scores (AR2 = .047, p > .05). The interaction term added in step three did significantly and positively contribute to the prediction of APS scores (AR2 = .063, p < .01), explaining an additional 6% ofthe variance in APS scores. As hypothesized, results showed that attachment bonds to the non-diagnosed parent moderated the relationship between attachment bonds to the diagnosed parent and APS scores, t(96) = -2.726, p < .01. The significant interaction effect was plotted as advised by Aiken and West (1991) to help interpret the interaction (see Figure 2). Data met regression assumptions regarding normality, linearity, and homoscedasticity. This interaction demonstrated that high indexes of attachment to the diagnosed parent are associated with lower levels of symptomatolgy and that this relationship is more pronounced when strong attachment bonds with the non-diagnosed parent also exist. 52 Table 4 Summary of Hierarchical llagression Analysis for Attachment Bonds PredictingAPS scores (N = 96). Variable B S_E B 13 Step 1 Gender of Diagnosed Parent -.24 .08 -.31** Parent Education .12 .07 .18 Number of Individual -.06 .03 -.23* Counseling Sessions Adolescent Psychotropic -. 13 . 13 -. 1 1 Medication use Step 2 Attachment bond to -.00 .00 .18 diagnosed parent Attachment bond to -.00 .00 -.20 non-diagnosed parent Step 3 Interaction -.00 .00 -.28** (attachment bond to diagnosed parent * attachment bond to non-diagnosed parent) Note. Adjusted R’ =. 194 for Step 1 (p < .001); A Adjusted RZ= .030 for Step 2 (us); A Adjusted 32 = .059 for Step 3 (p < .01). * p < .05; ** p <01 53 F_igure 2: Interaction for Regression Angaysis: Attachment Bond to Diagnosed Parent & Attachment Bond to Non-Diagnosed Parent as Predictor Variables and APS Scores as Criterion Variable 1.5 ‘1 0.5 - \ _+_ Low x2 0— "*a‘ -~~thx2 -0.5 — Low x1 High x1 Note. X1 = Attachment Bond to Diagnosed Parent, X2 = Attachment Bond to Non- Diagnosed Parent Regression Analyses: Contributions of Parental Attachment Bonds to the Prediction of Social Support Competence It was hypothesized that after controlling for selected demographic variables, indexes of secure parent-adolescent attachment would be Significantly related to each measure of social support competence. To test this hypothesis, three separate regression analyses were conducted. For each regression, main effects and interaction effects were tested using a three-step regression procedure. At the first step of each regression, selected demographic variables were entered as covariates. Attachment measures were entered at step two. Each measure of social support competence (PSS, SSE, and FNE) represented the dependent variable, respectively. Prior to testing the interaction term, 54 indexes of attachment to the diagnosed parent and to the non-diagnosed parent were appropriately “center ” as advised by Aiken & West (1991). Results of these regression analyses are presented in Table 5. Attachment Bonds and Perceived Social Support. The covariate block alone contributed 27% to the overall variance in PSS scores (R2 = .275, p > .001). While attachment bond to the diagnosed parent, t(96) = 3.44, p < .01, was significantly and positively predictive of PS S, no significant main effect was found for attachment bond to the non-diagnosed parent, t_(96) = - 1.08, p > .05. When entered as a block, attachment variables explained an additional 9% of the variance in PSS scores beyond that explained by the covariates alone, F (2,79) = 5.9, p < .01. The entry of the interaction term at the third step was not significant in enhancing the prediction of perceived social support. A_ttachment Bonds and Social Self-Efflcaazy. The covariate block alone contributed almost 18% of the overall variance in SSE scores (32 = .177, p <.01). Parental attachment bonds made significant individual contributions to the variance in SSE scores accounting for an additional 12% of the variance in SSE scores (AR2 = .126, p 5 .001). However, while attachment bond to the diagnosed parent, t(96) = 3.70, p < .001, was significantly and positively predictive of SSE, no significant main effect was found for attachment bond to the non-diagnosed parent, t_(96) = -1.613, p > .05. The inclusion of the interaction term was not significant in enhancing the prediction of SSE. A_ttachment Bonds and Fear of Negative Evaluation. The covariate block did make a significant contribution to the overall explained variance in FNE scores 55 (R2 = .123, p < .05), explaining 12% of the overall variance. However, after covariates were controlled, the additional predictors did not make significant, unique contributions to FNE. Table 5 Summary of Hierarchical Regression Analysis for Attachment Bonds Predicting Socia_l Competence scores (N = 96). Variable 3 Si: 3 B Perceived Social Support Step 1 Gender of Diagnosed Parent -2.1 1.63 -. 13 Parent Education -5.30 1.47 -.37** Number of Individual -.77 .56 -. 13 Counseling Sessions Adolescent. Medication Use -12.59 2.75 -.49** Step 2 Attachment bond to .15 .04 .33** diagnosed parent Attachment bond to -.03 .04 -.11 non-diagnosed parent Step 3 Interaction .OO .00 . 1 57 (attachment bond to diagnosed parent * attachment bond to non-diagnosed parent) 56 (table continues) Variable .3. SE B B Social Self Efficacy Step 1 Gender of Diagnosed Parent 15.18 5.05 .32** Parent Education -9.04 4.56 -.21 Number of Individual .48 1.73 .02 Counseling Sessions Adolescent Medication Use 4.52 8.5 .06 Step 2 Attachment bond to .52 .14 .38*** diagnosed parent Attachment bond to -.17 .11 -.17 non-diagnosed parent Step 3 Interaction -.OO .00 -.07 (attachment bond to diagnosed parent * attachment bond to non-diagnosed parent) Fear of Negative Evaluation Step 1 Gender of Diagnosed Parent -1.31 1.92 -.07 Parent Education -3.25 1.73 -.21 Number of Individual -.25 .66 -.04 Counseling Sessions Adolescent Medication Use 5.79 3.24 .21 Step 2 Attachment bond to -.O4 .05 -.09 diagnosed parent (table continues) Variable loo If? It: a Attachment bond to -.07 .04 -.21 non-diagnosed parent Step 3 Interaction -.00 .00 -. 13 (attachment bond to diagnosed parent * attachment bond to non-diagnosed parent) Nata. PSS: Adjusted R2 =.239 for Step 1(p < .001); A Adjusted 32: .08 for Step 2 (p <.Ol); A Adjusted 32 = .012 for Step 3 (ns). SSE: Adjusted 32 =. 136 for Step 1(p < .01); A Adjusted l_{2 = .114 for Step 2 (p <.001); A Adjusted 32 = .005 for Step 3 (ns). FNE: Adjusted 132 = .080 for Step 1 (p < .05); A Adjusted R’ = .00 for Step 2 (ns); A Adjusted 32 = .005 for Step 3 (ns). p < .05; ** p < .01; *** p_< .001 Rggression Analysis: Contributions of Perceived Social Support, Social Self-Efficacy, aad Fear of Negative Evalaation to the Prediction of Adolescenit Affective Psychopathology. Tables 6, 7 and 8 present the results of these analyses. As hypothesized, regression analyses demonstrated that after controlling for demographic variables, perceived social support was significantly and positively predictive of adolescent affective psychopathology, t(96) = 2.80, p < .01. Results also supported the hypothesis that fear of negative evaluation (reverse coded) was significantly and positively predictive of APS scores, t(96) = 5.06, p < .001. Additionally, social self-efficacy was 58 significantly and positively predictive of adolescent affective symptomatology, t(96) = 1.90, p < .05. Table 6 Summary of Hierarchical Regession Analysis for Perceived chal Sapport Predicting APS scores (N = 96). Variable B SE_B B Perceived Social Support Step 1 Gender of Diagnosed Parent -.24 .07 -.31*** Parent Education . 12 .07 .17 Number of Individual -.06 .02 -.23** Counseling Sessions Adolescent Medication Use -.13 .13 -.11 Step 2 PSS .01 .00 .33** Note. *p < .05; ** p < .01; *** p_< .001 Table 7 Summary of Hierarchical Reggession Analysis for Social Self Efiicac_y Predicting APS scores (N = 96; Variable E SE B [3 Social Self Efficacy Step 1 Gender of Diagnosed Parent -.24 .07 -31“: 59 Parent Education Number of Individual Counseling Sessions Adolescent Medication Use Step 2 SSE .12 -.06 -.13 .00 .07 .02 .13 .00 .17 -.23** -.11 .22“ Note. *p < .05; ** p < .01; *** p_< .001 Table 8 Summary of Hierarchical Regression Analysis for Fear of Negative Eva_lu_ation Predicting APS scores (N = 96). Variable B SE_B 13 Fear of Negative Evaluation Step 1 Gender of Diagnosed Parent -.24 .07 -31*** Parent Education . 12 .07 .17 Number of Individual -.O6 .02 -.23** Counseling Sessions Adolescent. Medication Use -. 13 . 13 -.1 1 Step 2 FNE .02 .00 .48*** 60 Note. PSS: Adjusted R’ =.194 for Step 1(p < .001); A Adjusted R9: .074 for Step 2 (p <.01). SSE: Adjusted 32 = .194 for Step 1(p < .001); A Adjusted 32: .035 for Step 2 (p <.05); FNE: Adjusted 32 = .194. for Step 1 (p < .001); A Adjusted 32 = .213 for Step 2 (p < .001). * p < .05; "p < .01; *** p_< .001 Figure 3 presents a summary of the obtained coefficients representing relationships among all key model variables. 61 figure 3: Path Diang Presenting a Summary of the Obtained Standardized Coefficients for Tested Relationships AmongAll Proposed Model Variables. (-..23) V A (48mins) (.12) Nata. X1 = Attachment bond to diagnosed parent; X2 = attachment bond to non- diagnosed parent; PSS = perceived social support; SSE = social self efficacy; FNE = fear of negative evaluation, APS = adolescent psychopathology scale. Standardized coefficients were used. *p < .05; **p< .01; ***p < .001 Post Hoc Analyses The goal of the current study was to propose and test a mediational model for explaining the relationship between parental attachment bonds and adolescent affective symptomatology. In order to examine mediating effects, however, a predictive relationship between parental attachment bonds and APS scores was required. Surprisingly, parental attachment bonds did not predict APS scores for this study sample. Given this pivotal finding, an analysis of the proposed mediator model as initially proposed was unwarranted. Instead, post hoc analyses were conducted to explore whether the attachment construct as measured in this study was masking a relationship between the variables. Additionally, post hoc analyses were conducted to further test model variable relationships. One potential reason for the inability of the attachment measures to predict APS scores in this study involves the measurement of parental attachment bonds. The parental attachment bond construct utilized in this study contains three subscale components: trust, communication, and alienation. In the analyses presented in this chapter to this point, these subscales were summed to obtain a total parental attachment bond score. This aggregate score was used to test the study hypotheses. However, given that a relationship between attachment bonds and affective symptomatology has been well documented, it seemed worthwhile to explore the possibility that a relationship in fact existed but that using an aggregate score masked the relationship. Scale descriptives and intercorTelations among these components of attachment and key model variables are presented in Table 9. 63 _o. V a... .mo. V a... 6.8m 30353263; E8333: u mm< Hogs—gm uZEwoZ :0 Sum n mZm SomoEm toaqsm Ecom u mmm "tomazm Eoom “630on n mmn. 3:23 cowgwflnéo: on: 5.3 @2383: c2232: E68553 n mx 523:2? 3:88 nomocwflu of 5:.» 33688 5:22.: E0858»: _x 52222 5:28 nomocwmfiéo: of 5:: 3860mm: :ocmoEsEEoQ u Nx :ozmo_::EEoU 2:23 38:36 05 .23 33888 :2825888 n .x :ocmoEzEEoU .883 nomo:wm€-:o: 2: 5:5 3:38me .25 u NX 53:. 3:28 :30:me 05 5:5 @8383: 3?: n Ca 53,—. .3qu o_r eewa cor esON£ ...... o; *§—V. asmm. w_. 0—. o_. vo. _ox : ‘I III, II..lrrOr I) to o; sewn. avm. oo. oo. *eoo. mo. 0; N_. 0—. mo. o; {*mrN. no. iiVm‘! Nor in! *in o; aevv. co. *eov. no. G; aahv. *aoo. *aom. o; «cow. 0; **_o. to 1 N eemw. o; meg. «;.o o_.m vogw oNa __.o_ was: am .19! I..-tier, all -91-I..... ‘ 'ri . wan” wogkn Qth_ voAK. vmd; owé; vw4A” wmwmm mvfmm madam co co co co mo 00 co 00 mo co us. I.. ‘11.. li.‘n'lv £2 .3: M77. mmm mm: Q 8:222 _x 5:822 Nx :ozmggEEoU _x :o_8o_::EEoU NXHQCH _Vn.m=+H 22E; 4 .3 r ivrvir II? .0 :6 hi mozmcm> Eco—2 GM cc: €039.22 6:: 623358800 $2.5 mucom EoEcowz< .958: go mEocanoU 05 :8 228—28885 we: .mcoumgom Emwafim £882 a 2an 64 Post hoc analyses were conducted to explore relationships among the attachment components and APS scores. First, post hoc regression analyses were conducted to determine if the components of the parental attachment made unique and significant contributions to the prediction of APS scores when measured independently. To investigate this possibility, APS scores were regressed on each attachment component separately. To remain consistent with the model as initially proposed, each component was assessed in relation to the diagnosed parent as well as the non-diagnosed parent. Thus, six separate regression analyses were required using APS scores as the dependent variable in each. Main effects and interaction effects for each regression were tested using a three-step regression procedure. For each regression, the covariates were entered in the first step. At step two, trust, alienation, and communication were entered, respectively, as a block. The interaction term was included at step three. Again, as recommended by Aiken and West (1991), measures of trust, communication, and alienation were appropriately “centered” prior to conducting the analyses. Results of the three regressions are presented in Table 10. As already described in earlier analyses, the covariate block alone contributed 23% to the overall variance in APS scores (R2 = .232, p <.001). When trust was entered at step two, it also made a significant contribution to the variance in APS scores, accounting for an additional 5% of the variance in APS scores (AR2 = .05, p < .05). However, while trust associated with attachment bond to the diagnosed parent, t(96) = -2.53 3, p < .05, was significantly and positively predictive of APS scores, no significant main effect was found for trust associated with attachment bond to the non-diagnosed parent, t (96) = 1.268, p > .05. The 65 inclusion of the interaction term was not significant in enhancing the prediction of APS scores. No main effects or interaction effects were found when APS scores were regressed on communication scores. Given the particularly strong correlational relationships between trust and communication scores for the diagnosed parent (r = .91, p < .01) and for the non-diagnosed parent (r = .90, p < .01), post hoc partial correlations were computed to firrther examine the relationship between communication and APS scores. However, after controlling for trust scores associated with the diagnosed parent, partial correlation coefficients indicated that communication scores were no longer statistically related to APS scores (r = .3 5, p > .05). After controlling for trust scores associated with the non-diagnosed parent, communication scores were also statistically unrelated to APS scores (r = .22, p > .05). These findings fail to support the notion that quality of communication with parents is linearly related to risk for affective symptomatology. Results indicated that after controlling for covariates, alienation did not make a unique significant contribution to the prediction of APS scores. However, the inclusion of the interaction term for alienation was significant, t (96) = 2.512, p < .05, in enhancing the prediction of APS scores, which precludes the interpretation of main effects. The interaction term contributed an additional 6% to the overall variance explained. The significant interaction effect was plotted to help interpret the interaction (see Figure 4). Data met regression assumptions regarding normality, linearity and homoscedasticity, 66 Table 10 Summary of Hierarchical Remssion Analysis for Trust. Communication, and Alienation Predicting APS scores (N = 96; Variable E SE B B TRUST Step 1 Gender of Diagnosed Parent -.24 .07 -.32** Parent Education .12 .07 .18 Number of Individual -.05 .07 -.22* Counseling Sessions Adolescent Medication Use -.13 .13 .10 Step 2 Trust: Attachment bond -.01 .00 -.29* to diagnosed parent Trust: Attachment bond to .00 .00 .15 non-diagnosed parent Step 3 Trust Interaction .00 .00 .13 (Trust: Attachment bond to Diagnosed parent * Trust: Attachment Bond to non-diagnosed parent) COMMUNICATION Step 1 Demographic Variables 67 Step 2 Communication: Attachment -.01 bond to diagnosed parent Communication: Attachment bond 00 to non—diagnosed parent Step 3 Communication Interaction .00 (Communication: attachment bond to diagnosed parent * Communication: attachment bond to non-diagnosed parent) ALIENATION Step 1 Demographic Variables Step 2 Alienation: Attachment bond .00 to diagnosed parent Alienation: Attachment bond to .00 non-diagnosed parent Step 3 Alienation Interaction .00 (Alienation: attachment bond to diagnosed parent * Alienation: attachment bond to non-diagnosed parent) .00 .00 .00 .00 .00 .00 -.26* .11 .06 .01 -.05 .72* Note. Trust: Adjusted R2 =. 199 for Step 1(p < .001); A Adjusted Ll": .043 for Step 2 (p <.05); A Adjusted R” = .008 for Step 3 (ns). Communication: Adjusted R’ =.194 for Step 1(p < .001); A Adjusted E = .036 for Step 2 (ns); A Adjusted 32 = .009 for Step 3 (ns). Alienation: Adjusted 32 = .194 for Step 1 (p < .001); A Adjusted 32 = .068 for Step 2 (ns); A Adjusted 32 = .052 for Step 3 (ns). * p < .05; ** p < .01; *** p_< .001 68 Figure 4: Interaction Between Attachment Alienation Associated the Diagnosed Parent and Attachment Alienation Associated with the Non-Diagnosed Parent in Predicting APS Scores 2.6 2.5 - 2.4 - 2.3 " 0 Low x2 2.2 - flgh x2 APS Scores 2.1- 2.. 1.9 . Low x1 High x1 Alienation Three patterns are suggested by the significant interaction: 1) low alienation associated with the diagnosed parent is related to lower symptomatology when lower alienation scores associated with the non-diagnosed parent are reported; 2) in cases where alienation associated with the non-diagnosed parent is high but low levels of alienation associated with the diagnosed parent exist, slightly lower symptomatology levels occur; and 3) the greatest effect of alienation on symptomatology occurs when high levels of alienation are associated with both the diagnosed parent and the non-diagnosed parent. 69 DISCUSSION mm This chapter initially summarizes and interprets this study’s key findings. The ways in which the findings of this study advance research into risk for affective psychopathology among offspring of bipolar disordered parents are then highlighted. Where hypotheses of this study were not supported, possible explanations that draw upon existing research and theory are offered. Implications of this study’s findings for therapeutic intervention and prevention are also discussed. Finally, methodological limitations are noted and recommendations for future research are addressed. The purpose of this study was to advance understanding of risk for affective psychopathology among adolescent children of bipolar parents. Because specific mechanisms through which risk is heightened or attenuated are not well understood, the focus of this study was to identify and test a potentially useful mediational model. The model tested was premised on extant genetic and psychosocial literature indicating that both the quality of one’s relationship with parents influences one’s sense of social competency; this, in turn may buffer the negative effects of parental affective disturbance on offspring. Attachment theory served as the lens through which this topic was addressed because of its heuristic utility in explaining the processes linking relational competency and outcomes for at-risk offspring. Attachment security has repeatedly been associated with better outcomes for children of affectively disturbed parents. The present study tested a path model which posited that quality of attachment to parents influences one’s perception of available social support, self-efficacy, and fear of being evaluated by others. These elements of social competency, in turn, impact risk for 70 affective symptomatology according to the model proposed. Hypotheses were tested regarding specific relationships among parental attachment bonds, perceived social support, social self-efficacy, fear of negative evaluation, and adolescent affective symptomatology. Next, a regression model indicating that attachment bonds were unique, significant predictors of adolescent affective psychopathology was tested. Consistent with the notion that attachment security promotes a healthy sense of self and others, it was also hypothesized the data would reveal that secure attachment with the non—diagnosed parent would moderate the relationship between insecure attachment with the diagnosed parent and affective symptomatology. Furthermore, it was anticipated that perceived social support, social self-efficacy, and fear of negative evaluation would predict adolescent affective psychopathology and mediate the relationship between parental attachment bonds and affective disturbance. These hypotheses were motivated by research findings which suggested that elucidating the specific coping mechanisms offspring engage in when dealing with an afl‘ectively ill parent is an important step toward reducing risk and improving intervention programs for this at-risk population (Goodman & Gotlib, 1999; Hodgins, F aucher, Zarac, & Ellenbogen, 2002). Correlational Analyses The path model presented in the current study was based, in part, upon the assumption that parental attachment bonds would be significantly correlated with APS scores; however, the findings did not support this assumption. Additionally, correlational analyses provided only partial support for anticipated relationships among indexes of parental attachment, social support competency, and adolescent affective symptomatology. Indexes of attachment bonds to the diagnosed parent were significantly and positively correlated with perceived social support and social self-efficacy but were, 71 unexpectedly, unrelated to fear of negative evaluation. It was anticipated that indexes of parental attachment bonds with the non-diagnosed parent would likewise be correlated with each measure of social competency; however, findings again did not demonstrate correlational relationships among these variables. Indexes of perceived social support, social self-efficacy, and fear of negative evaluation measure distinct but related constructs; therefore, it is not surprising that they were all significantly and positively correlated with each other. Further, while perceived social support and fear of negative evaluation were correlated with affective symptomatology as anticipated, social-self efficacy was not. A more detailed discussion of findings related to the relationship between social self-efficacy and adolescent risk follows. Group Comparisons of Secure Parental Attachment_ Contrary to hypotheses, adolescents in this study reported stronger attachment bonds with the diagnosed parent than with the non-diagnosed parent. This appears to be the first study that directly compares parental attachment between these two groups. Related studies compare parental attachments of clinical versus non-clinical groups rather than differentiating between the diagnosed parent and the non-diagnosed parent. In research on attachment theory and parental affective disturbance, the focus has been primarily on mothers-child relations. However, the risk literature has documented the positive role of a supportive adult in increasing the resilience of children at risk for psychopathology (Garmezy, 1985; Werner & Smith, 1980). Including fathers in the sample of diagnosed parents and attending to the attachment relationship with the non- diagnosed spouse is somewhat unique and may serve to broaden and diversify understandings of resiliency and risk. 72 The expectation that attachment bonds with the non-diagnosed parent would be stronger than those with the ill parent were based on extant research with depressed and bipolar parents which indicate that affective disturbance compromises parenting abilities. It has been reported that affective illness reduces a parent’s dependablity, availablity, and consistency, and that mood and energy dysregulation makes it less likely the parent can generate the sustained, effortfirl behavior required to cultivate secure attachment bonds (Anthony, 1975; Zahn-Waxler, 1984). While it should be noted that stronger attachment bonds with the ill parent found in this study are only relative to the attachment bonds with the non-ill parent in this study, these findings are in line with the view that assumptions should not be made about attachment relationships based on diagnosis alone (Seifer, Sameroff, Dickstein, Keitner, & Miller, 1996). Methodological explanations for the difference in attachment bonds with each parent found here are also possible. That is, in this study sixty percent of the parents in the diagnosed-parent category were mothers. Most research findings have shown that adolescents describe themselves as closer to mothers than fathers (Pipp, Shaver, Jennings, Lamborn, & Fischer, 1985). Furthermore, there are indications that children who have an affectively ill parent may be more empathic and prosocial towards a diagnosed mother than a diagnosed father. It was reported that children of parents with depressed mood were more able to recognize their mothers’ unhappiness and describe their own feelings about their mothers’ moods (Solantaus-Simula, Punamaeki, & Beardslee, 2002). The authors suggested that differences such as a heightened awareness of, and dialogue about, their mothers’ feelings and their own feelings about the mother’s moods, in combination with the mothers’ tendency to more promptly validate their child’s responses might 73 reflect more emotionally close relationships with mothers. Similarly, in a study using a cross-sectional sample of adolescents investigating perceptions of attachments with parents, adolescents reported seeking their mothers more than their fathers in support seeking situations and reported a higher quality of affect toward their mothers than fathers (Paterson, Field, & Pryor, 1994). The authors suggested that there may be more to the higher utilization of mothers than simply the fact that she is an easier person to communicate with. Fathers appeared to be perceived as less emotionally involved and thus not as equipped as mothers to offer appropriate comforting. Additionally, bipolar parents who have spouses or who are in close contact with family members represent only a portion of bipolar parents. Many parents with BPD are unmarried, divorced, or alienated from their families due to the strain the symptoms of the illness can place on relationships. It has been reported that a combination of parental affective illness and divorce is associated with poor outcome in the offspring of affectively ill adults (Beardslee et al., 1993). The very nature of the two-parent household required for inclusion in this study suggests that these diagnosed parents may have higher levels of support than others. Given that support from the non-ill parent has been linked to better outcomes for persons with affective disorders (Prince & Jacobson, 1995), it is possible that the diagnosed parents in this study were less severely impacted by their illness than those represented in other studies in this area, making them more able to foster strong attachment relationships with their adolescent offspring. Finally, research has also documented that more severe parental symptomatology may support an understanding of the parent as “ill” (Rutter, 1990). Offspring may be better able to understand and accept mood variability when it is pronounced. Similarly, 74 offspring may find less severe symptomatology more complicated and confirsing. While some information was obtained on the nature of the illness and treatment, it is beyond the scope of this study to be able to paint a clear picture of whether the parent could be classified as severely affected. Mal Attachment Bonds Predicting Adolescent Affective Psychopatholgy The data did not support the key assumption for this study that parental attachment bonds would make significant and unique contributions to the prediction of adolescent affective psychopathology. This result is particularly surprising given the extent of empirical and theoretical support for a relationship between these variables (Allen, 1999; Armsden & Greenberg, 1987; Engels, F inkenauer, Meeus, & Dekovic, 2001; Papina & Roggmann, 1992). The present study was designed in accordance with longitudinal research which demonstrates that insecure attachment predicts psychopathology (Kenny et. al., 1998; Papini & Roggman, 1992). Research also suggests that insecure attachment can be interpreted as a sequela of more longstanding psychiatric problems (e. g., depression) (Allen, Hauser, & Borman-Spurell, 1996). In the present study, long-term psychiatric difficulties were not assessed. However, indexes of adolescent affective symptomatology and information on their psychiatric history were obtained. Interestingly, overall the participants had slightly lower levels of symptomatology than might be expected. It should be noted that a slightly restricted range was found among APS scores, making it more difficult to detect a statistically significant effect. Furthermore, to date, published IPPA-R scores for non-clinical samples of adolescents are not available. However, a comparison of how parental attachment scores fiom the current study compare to those from a non-clinical sample 75 could serve as a basis for developing more sophisticated research questions concerning the nature of the relationship between attachment and risk for psychopathology. Another explanation for the unsubstantiated hypothesis regarding attachment bonds and APS scores is the idea that the conceptualization of the parent-child relationship may change with recovery for both the parent and the child (Cummings & Cicchetti, 1990). That is, perhaps the higher functioning sample represented here participated because they were able to do so — because they were “recovering.” However, at this time it is unclear where study participants rank in terms of recovery. The lack of a significant association between parental attachment bonds and APS scores may be a result of the study design. Initially, the three components of parental attachment bonds (e. g., trust, communication, and alienation) were aggregated to indicate the relative degree of attachment security with each parent. However, measuring a multidimensional construct in aggregate can mask important information about their unique contributions. Indeed, when the components of attachment were treated as focal in post hoc analyses, relationships with APS scores were found. Specifically, trust associated with the diagnosed parent predicted adolescent affective symptomatology, as did the interaction between alienation associated with the diagnosed parent and alienation associated with the non-diagnosed parent. These findings are consistent with research and theory that stresses the multidimensional character of attachment and those which emphasize the importance of mutual understanding and respect as well as feelings of anger and isolation (Armsden & Greenberg, 1987; Parkes & Stevenson-Hinde, 1982). However, the absence of a relationship between the communication subscale and affective symptomatology warrants comment. For the current study, communication and 76 trust scores were very highly correlated, which suggests that measurement errors may have occured. However, it is curious that when the relationship between communication and APS scores was tested after controlling for the effects of trust, no relationship emerged. Perhaps communication and trust both tap a construct unrelated to affective symptomatology. Furthermore, communication and APS scores may have a curvilinear relationship. Additional research may help clarify how quality of communication with parents - so often idiosyncratic during adolescence — impacts risk for offspring who manage a parent who vacillates between withdrawal and excessive, tangential, or unclear communication. Lastly, while direct relationships between composite attachment scores and affective symptomatology were not found, findings did support the hypothesis that attachment to the non-diagnosed parent moderated the relationship between attachment with the diagnosed parent and APS scores. Interestingly, high levels of parental attachment with the diagnosed parent were associated with lower levels of symptomatology, and this relationship was found to be more pronounced when strong attachment bonds with the non-diagnosed parent also existed. This finding is in line with Bowlby’s theory and the wide body of subsequent research that emphasizes the protective firnction of strong parental attachment bonds during adolescence. Parental Attachment Bonas Predictingimial Support Competency Overall, attachment bonds with the diagnosed parent were stronger predictors of social support competence than were attachment bonds with the non-diagnosed parent in this study. Attachment bonds with the diagnosed parent predicted perceived social support and social self-efficacy as expected, but did not predict fear of negative 77 evaluation. Surprisingly, attachment bonds with the non-diagnosed parent did not predict any measure of social support competency. In a closely related study, the mediational role of social skills and relational competence on the relationship between parental attachment and adolescent’s emotional adjustment was tested (Engels et. al., 2001). In that study, 12-14 year old participants were analyzed separately from 15-18 year old participants. For the 12—14 year old age group, no effects of parental attachment on social skills and relational competence emerged. However, in the 15-18 year old age group, parental attachment was moderately related to social skills and this, in turn, affected non-familial relational competence. These findings suggest that knowledge of the underlying developmental processes is critical to understanding risk for the development of psychopathology in offspring. In the current study, age differentiations were not made, which may partially explain why some of the expected relationships among parental attachment bonds and social competency measures were not found. Moreover, while attachment bonds and social competency are linked empirically and theoretically as reviewed earlier, findings from the current study are also in line with those which emphasize the multidimensional nature of the social competency construct; this study was likely affected by the measurement challenges this construct presents (Engels et. al., 2001; Sarason & Sarason, 1985). Social Support Competency Predicting AdolescenL Affective Pasachcmathology As expected, perceived social support, social self-efficacy, and fear of negative evaluation all predicted adolescent affective symptomatology in this study. These findings are consistent with previous research demonstrating that higher levels of social 78 competency may reduce risk for affective psychopathology (Stefos, Bauwens, Staner, Pardoen, & Mendlewics, 1996; Johnson, Winett, Beyer, Greenhouse, & Miller, 1999). Findings are compatible with the notion that “competency” refers to both a set of interpersonal relationship skills and experiences and the beliefs [about the self and others] that prompt one to employ those skills to recruit social support. Furthermore, these findings specify elements of social support competence that guide the adolescent to organize his or her social world in such a way that risk for affective disturbance. It is noteworthy that FNE scores predicted symptomatology but were surprisingly unrelated to parental attachment bonds in this study. This finding is in line with the developmental notion that peer relationships become increasingly influential during adolescence, and their influence, relative to that of parents, may fluctuate according to developmental transitions. For the adolescent offspring of a bipolar parent, his/her perception of the negative evaluations of others may impact their affective state, but it may be that their primary sense of how others are negatively evaluating them is linked more directly to peer attachment bonds than parental attachment bonds. Data also lend support to conclusions drawn in the single study reviewed earlier that directly assessed personal and social resources among offspring of bipolar parents (Pellegrini et. al., 1986). Findings from that study linked psychiatric well-being with a high quality of social resources, sense of control, and high self-esteem. While the social competency construct measured in that study was operationalized differently than in the present study, conclusions about the importance of social support both within and beyond the family environment for children affected by parental BPD are analogous. 79 IanLcations for IL’tervention and Prevention Clinical and preventive approaches that target offspring of parents with bipolar disorder have been slow to develop relative to advancements in the understanding of risk. While programs exist for offspring who present for psychiatric treatment, preventive interventions implemented prior to the onset of offspring illness are rare. Nevertheless, there are indications that interventions designed to reduce risk prior to the onset of symptoms are effective (Beardslee, Versage, Velde, Swatling, & Hoke, 2002; Rutter, 1990). Such interventions largely rely upon family-based psychoeducational strategies to modify risk factors and promote offspring resiliency. Risk factors targeted include dysfunctional parenting practices, family communication problems, misunderstanding about the illness, and self-blame and guilt (Beardslee et. al., 1997; Chang & Steiner, 2003). There are indications that programs aimed at developing the adolescents’ skill at accessing and utilizing social support (Beardslee et. al., 1997; Garmezy & Masten, 1990) promote resiliency. Data from the current study may inform clinicians and researchers involved in intervention and prevention programs in two main ways. First, the unanticipated finding that adolescents were more closely attached to the diagnosed parent than to the non- diagnosed parent has implications for family-based interventions. Clinicians should be cautious of making assumptions that diagnostic classification alone implies dysfirnctional attachment. Rather, clinicians and researchers should closely examine the specific qualities of the parent-child relationships within the family (e.g., degree of mutual understanding and respect, interpersonal connectedness) that may serve to buffer the 80 negative effects of the illness. This finding, coupled with the finding that a high level of attachment with each parent is associated with lower levels of symptomatology supports the notion that family interventions aimed at strengthening parent-child and marital relationships are worthwhile. Failure to take into account the potentially protective function of these relationships and incorporate this knowledge into treatment and preventive interventions would hinder progress in this area. Second, findings that link social‘competency to affective symptomatology provide useful information on the specific qualities that clinicians and other professionals could target when designing psychosocial interventions. That is, results suggest that it would be beneficial to focus on the adolescent’s ability to accurately perceive support when it is available, to effectively utilize relational support, and to develop optimistic attitudes and realistic appraisals of other’s opinions about the self. It is noteworthy that the number of individual counseling sessions the adolescent reported in this study was significantly and negatively associated with adolescent affective symptomatology. Participation in sixteen or more individual counseling sessions was associated with lower symptomatology than participation in five or fewer sessions. A literature review revealed no studies that address the role that length of counseling involvement plays for this specific population. Thus, it appears that data from this study may provide a starting point for future investigations of the benefit of long- term counseling versus more short-term participation for offspring of affectively- diagnosed parents. Lastly, the children in the current study appear to represent a healthier subset of at-risk offspring than was expected given statistics on the prevalence of disturbance 81 among offspring of affectively disordered parents. It has been noted that recognizing resilient qualities may serve as one fruitful model for the development of preventive- intervention programs (Beardslee & Wheelock, 1991). Data from the current study could be used to inform such future research. Limitatipns This study contains several noteworthy limitations. First, interpretations are limited because of the current impossibility of statistically controlling for genetic heritability; in this study risk was assumed because the child is both biologically related to the diagnosed parent and grew up in an environment where parental BPD existed. At this time there is no known way of establishing whether an offspring carries a specific genetic predisposition (e. g., carries a gene) for affective disturbance. Research is underway to isolate genetic material; until such evidence becomes available, current researchers in this area emphasize psychosocial factors when establishing risk (Hodgins, F aucher, Zarac, & Ellenbogen, 2002). Similarly, factors related to parental BPD, such as the timing and severity of episodes, treatment success, and life events undoubtedly impact the effect of the illness on offspring, as do numerous offspring characteristics like temperament, negative life events, and personality variables. Sample and instrument problems exist as well. Findings are based upon self- report instruments, as has most of the research of this type. Assessments of how people see themselves may not be as beneficial as methodologies without self-interpretation (e. g., experimental models, objective accounts). However, regarding the assessment of parental attachment bonds, it has also been noted that the use of a self-report instrument, rather than an observational procedure, could tap not only behavioral elements of 82 adolescents’ support seeking, but also the cognitive expectations of attachment figures (Bretherton, 1985). The use of self-report in the current study reflects the view that attachment represents aspects of a relationship from the point of view of one person in the dyad, in this case, the adolescent (Hinde, 1982). There could also be respondent bias in terms of those who choose to participate in the study and those who returned questionnaires. For example, depressed persons have a lack of energy and often avoid social contact, which may make them less likely respondents. Thus, the sample may be biased with an overrepresentation of individuals who experience fewer depressive symptoms. Further, it has been established that during periods of mania, in contrast to periods of depression, individuals exhibit less insight into their own behaviors. Hence, interpretations of results do not preclude the possibility that some of the responses to questionnaires may have been a consequence of the adolescent’s psychopathology. 1 Research also suggests that certain qualities of the parent’s disorder impact offspring risk (e. g., age of onset and diagnosis, required hospitalizations, medication) by way of their effect on parenting (Sameroff, 1987). Contradictory evidence suggests that clinical variables do not necessarily significantly predict outcomes for the diagnosed parent or offspring (Stefos, Staner, Pardoen, & Mendlewicz, 1996), and by extension, parenting. Findings fi'om the present study are more in line with the latter argument. That is, clinical variables associated with the parent’s illness were not associated with adolescent affective symptoms. However, the effect of these illness characteristics on attachment relationships with either parent is unclear in the current study. For example, while parents reported information on the amount of medication they received, the scope 83 of this study does not allow for a determination of the effect of that medication on the illness or on the quality of the parent-child attachment relationship. Additionally, there are indications that when a family is selected for inclusion in a research study of this kind where one parent has an affective disorder, there are likely to be other disorders that will have an impact on the child and on family functioning (Beardslee & Wheelock, 1997). Factors such as comorbid disorders in the diagnosed parent, marital discord, or disorders in the non-diagnosed parent were not controlled for and could have affected results in this study. Recommendations for Future Researc_h The findings of unexpected relationships in this study and the design limitations noted that may explain these findings provide direction for future research. That is, while the need for process-oriented models that elucidate mechanisms of risk for offspring remains strong, the complexity of factors influencing offspring risk makes testing such path models difficult at this time. Simplified models that investigate dyadic mechanisms that reduce or elevate risk would be beneficial in the effort to develop more elaborate path models. Nevertheless, findings from the current study do provide some specific direction for research on this topic. The finding that attachment bonds with the diagnosed parent were stronger than those with the non—diagnosed parent warrants additional study. Specifically, determining the prevalence of attachment classifications (e.g., secure versus insecure) among these groups would enable researchers to use established attachment theory principles to interpret the underlying processes at work. Classifications would also permit comparisons among data and would firrther equip clinicians to develop theoretically and 84 empirically-based interventions given significant advances made recently in the application of attachment theory principles (Cassidy & Shaver, 1999; Kobak, 1999; Liddle & Schwartz, 2002). Furthermore, Bowlby’ s idea that securely attached individuals are more likely to offer support when they sense the attachment figure needs it could be tested and applied to preventive-interventions if attachment classifications were available. Indexes of parental attachment bonds and adolescent symptomatology in the current study indicate that researchers should examine the extent to which or level at which a diagnosed parent engages in the negative risk behavior thought to characterize risk and not merely whether the parent is depressed or manic. Findings fi'om the current study are in line with those which advocate for multidimensional indicators of risk (Goodman & Gotlib, 1999; Rutter, 1987; Sameroff et al., 1987). Findings from the current study also suggest that longitudinal, developmentally informed studies are needed. The current study grouped together children whose ages vary widely which likely masked important developmental influences on the relationships tested. Conclusions drawn from studies of children in one developmental stage cannot be presumed to generalize to another period in children’s lives. Findings from the current study should alert researchers to the importance of attending to the developmental issues of various ages and avoid grouping children or adolescents in this way. The design of the current study did allow for various levels of symptomatology among the adolescents to be represented. This design was motivated by the fact that a significant number of children manifest resiliency in the face of the stress of having a parent diagnosed with bipolar disorder. These offspring provide examples of how to 85 survive stressful relational circumstances. It is as important to be able to understand and recognize risk and resiliency as it is to recognize pathology. This idea is in line with researchers who argue that it is important to understand that non-ill, resilient children exist in samples of high-risk children and to be able to characterize and understand their resiliency is as important as it is to recognize impairment (Richmond & Beardslee, 1988). Finally, although path-analytic techniques cannot rule out alternative causal explanations or reciprocal effects, testing mediating effects offers potential for elucidating important information about links between the model variables. Research aimed at elucidating the adaptive function of attachment bonds as a means of facilitating the development of social competence and preventing affective psychopathology are crucial to diminishing offspring risk. 86 APPENDIX A RECRUITING NOTIFICATION CALL FOR RESEARCH PARTICIPANTS Description of the study: This study investigates resiliency in adolescent offspring of parents diagnosed with bipolar disorder. Participants needed: Parents and their adolescent(s) Requirements for participation: 1) You must be the biological parent of an adolescent (age 12-17) and you must have been diagnosed with bipolar disorder at some time during the adolescent’s lifetime, 0R 2) You must be the spouse of a parent who has been diagnosed with bipolar disorder and you must have lived with the adolescent for the majority of the adolescent’s lifetime. (The diagnosed parent must be the biological parent of the adolescent.) If you would like more information on this study or you might be interested in participating, please contact the principal investigator, Tracy Simko, at 7 7 3-665-0003 or by email at tracys@ix.netcom.com. 87 APPENDIX B REMINDER CARD FOR MATERIALS NOT RECEIVED Date Dear Research Participant, Several weeks ago you consented to participate in a research study on parental bipolar disorder. Your materials have not yet been received. If you have already returned the materials, please disregard this notice. Ifyou have not yet returned the materials and would like assistance or more time to complete them, please contact the principal investigator, Tracy Simko, at 773-665-0003. Your participation in the study is valuable; it will contribute to the understanding of bipolar disorder and positive parent-child relationships. Thank you very much for your time. Tracy Simko, M.A. Principal Investigator 773-665-0003 2123 N. Magnolia Ave. Chicago, IL 60614 tracys@ix.netcom.com 88 APPENDIX C PARENT DEMOGRAPHIC QUESTIONNAIRE Code Was the adolescent adopted? Yes No What is your relationship to the adolescent? Annual household income (optional): < $20,000 $20,000 - $40,000 $40,000 - $60,000 $60,000 - $80,000 $80,000 — $100, 000 100,000 + How many others in the adolescent’s family (immediate or extended) have been diagnosed with an affective disorder (e. g., depression, bipolar disorder)? The following questions refer to the diagnosed parent: Date of birth Highest level of education received: Diagnostic/treatment history: Age of onset Age of diagnosis Approximate number of affective episodes in lifetime Treatment history: Number of hospitalizations 0 __ 1-3 4 + _ 89 Parent Demographic Questionnaire pg 2 Medication Have you taken medication for bipolar disorder? Y N If yes, Antidepressant _ Dates Antianxiety _ Dates Antipsychotic __ Dates Adolescent diagnostic/treatment history: Age of onset Age of diagnosis Approximate number of affective episodes in lifetime Treatment history: Number of hospitalizations 0 __ 1-3 _ 4 + __ Medication My child has taken medication for an affective disorder _ Antidepressant __ Dates Antianxiety __ Dates Antipsychotic _ Dates My child has not taken medication for an affective disorder _ 90 l I _ ,,.__ .._.._ a u" a, Ari APPENDIX D ADOLESCENT DEMOGRAPHIC QUESTIONNAIRE Code Date of Birth: Sex: Male Female Current Grade Level: Ethnicity: Afiican—American American Indian Asian Caucasian Hispanic Other, please specific Have you ever received any of the following counseling services? Individual Yes No If Yes, approximately how many? 1-5 6-1 5 16-30 3 0+ Group Yes No If Yes, approximately how many? 1-5 6-15 16-30 30+ Family Yes No If Yes, approximately how many? 1-5 6-15 16-30 30+ Have you ever been hospitalized for psychiatric/psychological services? Yes No if Yes, approximately how many times? 1-2 3-10 10+ Have you ever taken psychotropic medication? Yes No If Yes, Antidepressant __ Dates Antianxiety _ Dates Antipsychotic _ Dates 91 Adolescent Demographic Questionnaire pg. 2 Have you been arrested for a misdemeanor crime? Yes No Have you been arrested for a felony crime? Yes No Have you used illicit drugs? Yes No 92 Parent Demographic 93 Parent Demographic Page 2 94 _--.......o .i-'.h . u Adol Demographic 95 Adol Demo page 2 96 aps 97 APS 98 APS 99 h I..“ .l-.‘ ill .t-n. 1‘" IPPA 100 IPPA 101 PSS 102 PSS 103 S S P 104 SSE 105 106 REFERENCES Aiken, L. & West, S.G. (1991 ). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications, Inc. Ainsworth, M.D., Blehar, M.C., Waters, E., & Wall, 8. (1978). Patterns of attachment: A psychological study of the strange situation. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Ainsworth, M. S. (1989). Attachments beyond infancy. American Psychologist. 44, 709-716. Allen, J. P. & Land, D. Attachment in adolescence. (1999). In J. Cassidy & P.R. Shaver (Eds), Hancfiook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications 07p. 319-335). New York: Guilford Press. Allen, J., Hauser, S. & Bonnan-Spurrell, E. (1996). Attachment theory as a framework for understanding sequelae of severe adolescent psychopathology: An 11 year follow-up study. Journal of Consulting & Clinical Psycholog, 64, 254-263. Anthony, El, (1975). The influence of a manic-depressive environment on the developing child. In B]. Anthony & T. Benedek (Eds), Depression and human existence. Boston: Little, Brown. Armsden, G.C., & Greenberg, MT. (1987). The Inventory of Parent and Peer Attachment: Individual differences and their relationship to psychological well-being in adolescence. Joumal of Youth and Adolescence, 16, 427-454. Armsden, G.C., McCauley, E., Greenberg, M.T., Burke, P.M., & Mitchell, J .R., (1990). Parent and peer attachment in early adolescent depression. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 18(6), 683-697. Armsden, G.G. (1986). Attachment to parents and peers in late adolescence: Relationships to affective status, self-esteem and coping with loss, threat and challenge. Dissertation Abstracts International, 4 7(4-B), 1751-1752 Armsden, G., McCauley, E., Greenberg, M., Burke, P., (1990). Parent and peer attachment in early adolescent depression. Journal of A bnormal Child Psychology, 18(6), 683-697. Baron, RM., & Kenny, DA. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51(6), 1 173-1 182. 107 ~‘ A.._v.' Beardslee, W.R., Versage, E.M., & Gladstone, TR. (1998). Children of affectively ill parents: A review of the past 10 years. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 3 7(1 1), 1 134-1141. 1‘“: Beardslee, W. R. & Podorefsky, D. (1988). Resilient adolescents whose parents have serious affective and other psychiatric disorders: Importance of self-understanding and relationships. American Journal of Psychiatry, 145(1), 63-69. Beardslee, W., Wright, W., Salt, P., & Drezner, K. (1997). Examination of children’s responses to two preventive intervention strategies over time. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 36(2), 196-204. Beardslee, W. & Wheetlock, I. (1994). Children of parents with affective disorders: Empirical findings and clinical implications. In W. Reynolds & H.F. Johnston (Eds). Handbook of depression in children cmd adolescents. Issues in clinical child psychology, (pp. 463-479). Beck, A. (1967). Depression: Clinical, experimental, and theoretical aspects. New York: Harper & Row. Blain, M.D., Thompson, J.M., & Whiffen, VB. (1993). Attachment and perceived social support in late adolescence: The interaction between working models of self and others. Journal of Adolescent Research, 8(2), 226-241. Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss: Vol 1. Attachment, New York: Basic Books. Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and loss: Vol 2. Separation, New York: Basic Books. Bowlby, J (1980). Attachment and loss: Vol 3. Loss: Sadness and depression. New York: Basic Books. Bowlby, J. (1982). Attachment and loss: Vol. 1. Attachment, (2nd ed.). New York: Basic Books. Bowlby, J. (198 8). A secure base: Parent-child attachment and healthy human development. New York: Basic Books. Bretherton, E. (1985). Attachment theory: Retrospect and prospect. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 5 0(1-2), 3-35. Carlson, E. A, & Sroufe, L. (1995). Contribution of attachment theory to developmental psychopathology. In D. Cicchetti & D. Cohen (Eds), Developmental psychopathology, Vol. 1: Yheory and methods. Wiley series on personality processes. (pp. 581-617). New Yorlg NY,: John Wiley & Sons 108 Cassidy, J. (1994). Emotion regulation: Influences on attachment relationships. In N. Fox (Ed), The development of emotion regulation: Biological and behavioral considerations. Monographs for the Society for Research in Child Development, 59, (l - 2), 228-249. Cassidy, J., & Shaver, P. R. (1999). Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Cassidy, J. & Kobak, R. R. (1988). Avoidance and its relation to other defensive processes. In J. Belsky, & T. Neworski (Eds), Clinical implications of attachment. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Chang, K., Steiner, PL, Dienes, K., Adleman, N., & Ketter, T. (2003). Bipolar offspring: A window into bipolar disorder evolution. Biological Psychiatry, 53(11), 945- 951. Coble, H.M., Gantt, D., & Mallinckrodt, B. (1996). Attachment and social competencies. In GR Pierce et al, (Eds), Handbook of social support and the family (pp. 141-172). New York: Plenum. Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112(1), 155-159. Connolly, J .A. (1989). Social self efficacy in adolescence: Relations with self concept, social adjustment and mental health. Canadian Journal of Behavioral Science, 21, 259-269. Cummings, E.M. & Cicchetti, D. (1990). Toward a transactional model of relations between attachment and depression. In M.T. Greenberg, D. Cicchetti, & E.M. Cummings (Eds), Attachment in the preschool years: Theory, research, and intervention (pp. 339-3 72), University of Chicago Press. Cutrona, CE. (1982). Transition to college: Loneliness and the process of social adjustment. In LA. Peplau & D. Perlman (Eds), Loneliness: A sourcebook of current theory, research, and therapy (pp. 291-3 09). New York: Wiley. Cutrona, C.E., & Russel, D.W. (1987). The provisions of social relationships and adaptation to stress. In W.H. Hones & D. Perlman (Eds), Advances in personal relationships (pp. 37-67). Greenwich, CT: Jai Press. Cytryn, L., McKnew, D.H., Bartko, J.J., Lamour, M., & Hamovit, J. (1982). Offspring of parents with affective disorders. H. Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 21, 389-391. Davenport, Y.B., Adland, M.L., Gold, P.W., & Goodwin, F .K. (1979). Manic- 109 depressive illness: Psychodynamic features of multigenerational families. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 49, 24-35. Davenport, Y.B., Zahn-Waxler, C., Adland, M.L., & Mayfield, A. (1984). Early child-rearing practices in families with a manic-depressive parent. American Journal of Psychiatry, 141, 230-235. Downey, G., & Coyne, J .C. (1990). Children of depressed parents: An integrative review. Psychological Bulletin, 108(1), 50-76. Dozier, M., & Kobak, R., (1993). Psychophysiology in attachment interviews: Converging evidence for deactivating strategies. Annual Progress in Child Psychiatry and Child Development, 80-92. Dozier, M., Stovall, K.C., & Albus, KB. (1999). Attachment and psychopathology in adulthood, In J. Cassidy & P.A. Shaver (Eds), Hancfiook of Attachment (pp. 497-519). Ehrenberg, M., & Cox., A. (1991). The relationship between self efficacy and adolescents. Adolescence, 102, 361-374. Engels, R., Finkenauer, C., Meeus, W., & Dekovic, M. (2001). Parental attachment and adolescents’ emotional adjustment: The associations with social skills and relational competence. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 48(4), 428-439. Erickson, M. F., Sroufe, L. A., Egeland, B., & Byron, T. (1985). The relationship between quality of attachment and behavior problems in preschool in a high-risk sample. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 50(1-2),147-166 Fisher, L., Kokes, R.F., Cole, R.E., Perkins, P. M., & Wynne, LC. (1987). Competent children at risk: A study of well-functioning offspring of disturbed parents. In A.E. James, E. Cohler & J. Bertram (Eds), The invulnerable child. The Guilford psychiatry series. (pp. 211-228). NY: Guilford Press. Ford, ME. (1982). Social cognition and social competency in adolescence. Developmental Psychology, 18, 323-341. Furnham, A., & Argyle, M. (1981). Responses of four groups to difficult social situations. In M. Argyle, A. F umham, & J. Graham (Eds), Social situations. (pp.336- 344). Cambridge University Press. Gaensbauer, T.J., Harmon, R.J., Cytryn, L., & McKnew, DH. (1984). Social and affective development in infants with a manic-depressive parent. American Journal of Psychiatry, 141, 223-229. 110 Garmezy, N. (1989). The role of competence in the study of children and adolescents under stress. In B. Schneider & G. Attili, (Eds), Social competence in developmental perspective. NATO Advanced Science Institutes series. Series D: Behavioural and social sciences, 51, (pp. 25-39). Boston: Kluwer Academic Publishers. Garmezy, N., & Masten, A. (1990). The adaptation of children to a stressful world: Mastery of fear. In E.L. Arnold (Ed), Chilflrood stress. Wiley series in child and adolescent mental health, (pp. 460-47 3). New York: Wiley. Garmezy, N. & Masten, A. (1991). The protective role of competence indicators in children at risk. In M. E. Cummings and A. L. Greene (Eds), Life-span developmental psychology: Perspectives on stress and coping, 15, 151-174. New York: Wiley. Gershon, E., McKnew, D., Cytryn, L., Harnmovit, J ., Schreiber, J ., Hibbs, E., & Pellegrini, D. (1985). Diagnosis in school-age children of bipolar affective disorder parents and normal controls. Journal of Aflective Disorders, 8(3), 283-291. Goodman, S.H., & Gotlib, 1.]. (1999). Risk for psychopathology in children of depressed mothers. Psychological Review, 106, 45 8-490. Greenberg, M.T., Seigal, J .M., & Leitch, CJ. (1983). The nature and importance of attachment relationships to parents and peers during adolescence. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 12, 373-386. Grigoroiu-Serbanescu, M., Christodorescu, D., Jipescu, I., Totoescu, A., Marinescu, E., & Ardelean, V. (1989). Psychopathology in children aged 10-17 of bipolar parents: Psychopathology rate and correlates of the severity of the psychopathology, Journal of Affective Disorders, 16, 167-179. Hammen, C., Burge, D., Burney, E., & Adrian, C. (1990). Longitudinal study of diagnosis in children of women with unipolar and bipolar affective disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry, 47, 1112-1 117. Hodgins, S., Faucher, B., Zarac, A., & Ellenbogen, M. (2002). Children of parents with bipolar disorder: A population at high risk for major affective disorders. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 11(3), 533-554. Izard, C. E, Haynes, O., Chisholm, G., & Baak, K. (1991). Emotional determinants of infant-mother attachment. Child Development, 62(5), 906-917. Johnson, S.L., Winett, C.A., Meyer, B., Greenhouse, W.J., & Miller, I. (1999). Social support and the course of bipolar disorder. Journal of A bnormal Psychology, 108 (4), 558-566. Kenny, M. (1987). The extent and firnction of parental attachment among first- year college students. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 16, 17-29. 111 I“. r l l: Kenny, M., Moilanen, D., Lomax, R, & Brabeck, M. (1993). Contributions of parental attachments to view of self and depressive symptoms among early adolescents. Journal of Early Adolescence, 13(4), 408-430. Kobak, R (1999). The emotional dynamics of disruptions in attachment relationships: Implications for theory, research, and clinical intervention, In J. Cassidy & P. Shaver (Eds), Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications, (up. 21.43) Kobak, R R, & Sceery, A. (1988). Attachment in late adolescence: Working models, affect regulation, and representations of self and others Child Development, 59, 135-146. Kobak, R, Sudler, N., & Gamble, W. (1991). Attachment and depressive symptoms during adolescence: A developmental pathways analysis. Development & Psychopathology. Vol 3(4), 461-474. Leary, M.R (1983). A brief version of the Fear of Negative Evaluation Scale. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 9, 371 -375. Lafley, HP. (1989). Family burden and family stigma in major mental illness. American Psychologist, 44(3 ), 556-560. Lewinsohn, R, Klein, D., & Seeley, J. (1995). Bipolar disorders in a community sample of older adolescents: prevelance, phenomenology, comorbidity, and course. Journal of American Academic of Child Adolescent Psychiatry, 34, 454-463. Lewis, F.M., Woods, N.F., & Ellison, B. (1983-1986). The Family Impact Study: The impact of cancer on the family. Grant R01-NU-01000, University of Washington, Division of Nursing, Public Health Service. Liddle, H., & Schwartz, S. (2002). Attachment and family therapy: The clinical utility of adolescent-family attachment research, Family Process. 41 (3 ), 455-476. Main, M. (1990). Cross-cultural studies of attachment organization: Recent studies, changing methodologies, and the concept of conditional strategies. Human Development, 33(1), 48-61. Mallinckrodt, B. (1992). Childhood emotional bonds with parents, development of adult social competencies, and availability of social support. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 39(4), 453-461. Matas, L., Arend, R., & Sroufe, A. (1978). Continuity of adaptation in the second year: The relationship between quality of attachment and later competence. Child Development. 49(3), 547-556. 112 “A. D‘A'r- ' / Mayo, J.A., O'Connell, R.A.., & O'Brien, J .D. (1979). Families of manic- depressive patients: effect of treatment. American Journal of Psychiatry, 136(12), 153 5- 1539. McKnew, D.H., Cytryn, L., Efron, A.M., Gershon, E.S., & Bunney, W.E. (1979). Offspring of patients with affective disorders. British Journal of Psychiatry, 134, 148- 1 52. Murray, C.J.L., & Lopez, AD. (1996) The global burden of disease: A comprehensive assessment of mortality and disability fiom diseases, injuries, and risk factors in 1990 and projected to 2020. Boston: Harvard University Press. O’Connell, R.A., Mayo, J .A., Eng, L.K., Jones, J .S., & Gabel, RM. (1985). Social support and long-term lithium outcome. British Journal of Psychiatry, 14 7, 272- 275. O’Connor, L., Berry, J ., Weiss, J ., & Gilbert, P. (2002). Guilt, fear, submission, and empathy in depression. Journal of Aflective Disorders, 71(1-3), 19-27. Oestman, M, & Kjellin, L. (2002). Stigma by association: Psychological factors in relatives of people with mental illness. British Journal of Psychiatry, 181(6), 494-498. Papini, D., & Roggrnan, L.. (1992). Adolescent perceived attachment to parents in relation to competence, depression, and anxiety: A longitudinal study. Journal of Early Adolescence, 12(4), 420-440. Parkes, C. M., & Stevenson-Hinde, J. (1982). The place of attachment in human behavior. New York: Basic Books. Patterson, J., Field, J ., and Pryor, J. (1994). Adolescents’ perceptions of their attachment relationships with their mothers, fathers, and fiiends Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 23, 579-600. Pellegrini, D., Kosisky, S., Nackman, D., Cytryn, L., McKnew, D.H., Gershon, E., Hamovit, J., & Cammuso, K. (1986). Personal and social resources in children of patients with bipolar affective disorder and children of normal control subjects. Annual Progress in Child Psychiatry & Child Development, 494-507. Pianta , R, Egeland, B., & Sroufe, L. (1990). Maternal stress in children’s development: Predictions of school outcomes and identification of protective factors. In J Rolf, A Masten, D. Cicchetti, K. Neuchterlen, & S. Weintraub (Eds), Risk and protective factors in the development of psychopathology (pp. 215-23 5). New York: Cambridge University Press. 113 Pipp, S., Shaver, P., Jennings, S., Lambom, S., & Fischer, K. (1985). Adolescents’ theories about the development of their relationships with parents. Journal of Personal & Social Psychology, 48, 991-10001. Pound, A. (1996). Parental affective disorder and childhood disturbance. In M. Goepfert & J. Webster (Eds), Parental psychiatric disorder: Distressed parents and their families. (pp. 201-218). Cambridge, England UK: Cambridge University Press. Prince, 8., & Jacobson, N. (1995). A review and evaluation of marital and family therapies for affective disorders. Journal of Marital Family Therapy, 21, 377-401. Procidano, M.E., & Heller, K. (1983). Measures of perceived social support from fiiends and family: Three validation studies. American Journal of Community Psychology, 11, 1-23. Radke-Yarrow, M., Cummings, E.M., Kucynski, L., & Chapman, M. (1985). Patterns of attachment in two- and three-year olds in normal families and families with parental depression. Child Development, 56, 884-889. Reynolds, W.M. (1998). Adolescent Psychopathology Scale: Psychometric and technical manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. Reynolds, W. M. (1993). Self-report methodology. In T. Ollendick & M. Ersen (Eds). Handbook of child and adolescent assessment: General psychology series, 1 6 7 (pp. 98-123). Boston: Allyn & Bacon, Inc. Reynolds, W.M. (1992). Introduction to the nature and study of internalizing disorders in children and adolescents. In W.M. Reynolds (Ed), Internalizing disorders in children and adolescents (pp. 1-18). Wiley Publications: US. Rice, K.G., Cunningham, T.I., & Mitchell, B.Y. (1997). Attachment to parents, social competence, and emotional well-being: A comparison of Black and White late adolescents. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 44(1), 89-101. Richmond, J ., & Beardslee, W. (1988). Resiliency: Research and practical implications for pediatricians. Journal of Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics, 9(3), 1 5 7-163 . Rosenstein, D.S., & Horowitz, HA. (1996). Adolescent attachment and psychopathology. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 64(2), 244-253. Rush, A.J., Cain, J.W., Raese, J., Stewart, RS., Waller, D.A., & Debus, JD. (1991). Neurobiological bases for psychiatric disorders. In RN. Rosenberg (Ed), Comprehensive neurology (pp. 555-603.) New York: Raven Press. 114 Russell, D., Cutrona, E., & Yurko, K., (1984). Social and emotional loneliness: an examination of Weiss’s typology of loneliness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46(6), 1313-1321. Rutter, M. (1990). Some focus and process considerations regarding effects of parental depression on children. Developmental Psychology, 26, 60-67. Rutter, M. (1987). Psychosocial resilience and protective mechanisms. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 5 7, 316-331. Safran, J. (1990). Towards a refinement of cognitive therapy in light of interpersonal theory: 1. Theory. Clinical Psychology Review, 10, 87-106. Sarason, 1., Pierce, G., & Sarason, B. (1990). Social support and interactional processes: A triadic hypothesis. Journal of Social & Personal Relationships. 7, 495-506. Sarason, I; Sarason, B. Shearin, E. (1986). Social support as an individual difference variable: Its stability, origins, and relational aspects. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology. 50, 845-855. Sarason, B.R., Pierce, GR, Shearin, E.N., Sarason, I.G., Waltz, J., & Poppe, L. (1990). Perceived social support and working models of self and actual others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 273-287. Seifer, R, Sameroff, A, Dickstein, S., Keitner, G., & Miller, I. (1996). Parental psychopathology, multiple contextual risks, and one-year outcomes in children. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 25(4), 423 -43 5. Solantaus-Sirnula, T., Punamaeki, R, & Beardslee, W. (2002). Children’s responses to low parental mood. Balancing between active empathy, overinvolvement, indifference and avoidance. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 41(3), 2787-286. Sroufe, A., Schork, E., Motti, F., Lawroski, N., & LaFreniere, P. (1985). The role of affect in social competence. In C. Izard & J. Kagan (Eds), Emotions, cognition, and behavior, (pp. 289-319). Sroufe, LA. (1983). Infant-caregiver attachment and patterns of adaptation in preschool: The roots of maladaptation and competence. In M. Perlmutter (Ed), Minnesota symposium on child psychology, 16, (pp. 41-83). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Sroufe, L., Levy, A.K., & Carlson, E. (1998). Resilience in adolescence: Changing the odds. Paper presented at the biennial meeting of the Society for Research on Adolescence, San Diego, CA. 115 Stefos, G., Bauwens, F., Staner, L., Pardoen, D., & Mendlewics, J. (1996). Psychosocial predictors of major affective recurrences in bipolar disorder: a 4-year longitudinal study of patients on prophylactic treatment. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinaviaca, 93, 420-426. Todd, R D., Reich, W., Petti, T.A., Joshi, P., DePaulo, J .R., Numberger, J., & Reich, T. (1996). Psychiatric diagnoses in the child and adolescent members of extended families identified through adult bipolar affective disorder probands. Journal of American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 35(5), 664-671. Uchino, B.N., Cacioppo, J .T., & Kiecolt-Glaser, J. (1996). The relationship between social support and physiological processes: A review with emphasis on underlying mechanisms and implications for health. Psychological Bulletin, 119, 488- 531. Vertue, RN. (2003). From adaptive emotion to dysfirnction: An attachment perspective on social anxiety disorder. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 7(2), 170-191. Watson, D., & Friend, R (1969). Measurement of social-evaluative anxiety. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 33, 448-45 7. Werner, E., & Smith, R. (1980). An epidemiologic perspective on some antecendents and consequences of childhood mental health problems and learning disabilities. Annual Progress in Child Psychiatry & Child Development, 133-147. Weiss, RS. (1982). Attachment in adult life. C.M. Parkes & J. Stevenson-Hinde (Eds), The place of attachment in human behavior, New York: Basic Books. Weiss, RS. (1974). The provisions of social relationships. In Z. Rubin (Ed), Doing unto others (pp. 17-26). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Printice-Hall. Weissman, M., Leaf, P., & Tischler, G. (1988). Affective disorders in five United States communities. Psychological Medicine, 18, 141-153. Winemiller, D.R., Mitchell, M., Sutliff, J ., & Cline, DJ. (1993). Measurement strategies in social support: A descriptive review of the literature. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 49(5), 638-648. Zahn-Waxler, C., Cummings, E.M., McKnew, D.H., & Radke—Yarrow, M. (1984). Altruism, aggression, and social interaction in young children with a manic- depressive parent. Child Development, 55, 112-122. 116 lllllllllllllrilll