. “10.3.2.3 flow-v. .4 .. PE 9. i ...«....u..‘.uz.._.... mu. 3 . .5! « Hm . . , v ‘1‘ NWT? .. in“: , an .fflfiue»... 0. 3m.- . my 3‘ 3.. .1. .. mm}... :4. .. , )1 . : a 13.: r. “awning. laugh. a. . J ... 1...... .u‘ .1 . . {£155.}, 1. k '3. mitt“: .. if; .3: Liu.r..,i..f 1:. .I.).lt fir}. LIBRARY Michigan State U I'IIVG rSity dissertation entitled This is to certify that the AWARENESS IN GLOBAL VIRTUAL TEAMS: ITS ANTECEDENTS AND IMPLICATIONS presented by Chyng—Yang Jang has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for the Telecommunication, Information Ph. D. degree in Studies and Media ,1 / Major Profeséofr’s Signature I 2 // 2/0 ‘2 Date MSU is an Affirmative Action/Equal Opportunity Institution PLACE IN RETURN BOX to remove this checkout from your record. TO AVOID FINES return on or before date due. MAY BE RECALLED with earlier due date if requested. DATE DUE DATE DUE DATE DUE 6/01 c:/ClRC/DateDuo.p65-p.15 AWARENESS IN GLOBAL VIRTUAL TEAMS: ITS ANTECEDENTS AND IMPLICATIONS By Chyng-Yang Jang A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Telecommunication, Information Studies and Media 2003 ABSTRACT AWARENESS IN GLOBAL VIRTUAL TEAMS: ITS ANTECEDENTS AND IMPLICATIONS By Chyng-Yang Jang This study investigated the antecedents and impacts of awareness in global virtual teams. Awareness is a broad construct that reflects the extent to which people participating in a collaborative activity possess relevant information about aspects of their teammates or teammates’ work. The purpose of this study was to integrate the concept of awareness into the literature of work group study. Specifically, the effects of perceived task structure, communication frequency and groupware usage on perceived awareness were examined. In addition, the effects of awareness, along with task interdependence, on trust and solution agreement were also explored. Participants were recruited from universities in the United States, Mexico, and Russia to form 7 distributed engineering design teams. Each team consisted of students from 2 locations and was offered a set of communication tools, including an lSDN-based video conferencing system (VT EL), Microsoft Netmeeting, telephone, fax, a project-specific email account, and a web-based groupware tool known as TeamSCOPE. The results showed that perceived task interdependence and communication frequency had positive effects on perceived level of awareness. Additionally, the effect of communication frequency on perceived awareness was moderated by perceived task interdependence. Results also showed that participants’ reliance on real-time meetings with distant teammates for awareness was positively associated with increased perceived task interdependence and contributed to a higher level of perceived awareness. However, no effect of TeamSCOPE usage on perceived awareness was found. Further discussion on awareness demands is provided. Both perceived task interdependence and awareness were positively associated with trust. Multiple regression analysis showed that perceived awareness was the more significant predictor of trust. No effect was found between awareness and solution agreement. These results suggest that awareness is an important group process variable. It provides a link between task interdependence and trust. To stay updated with their distant teammates, virtual team participants need adequate media support to match their awareness demand. Organizations should reduce the barriers of using synchronous communication channels for virtual teams, especially for those working on high interdependent tasks. To my parents and my wife, whose love and support made this work possible. iv ACKNOWLEDGENTS To be able to complete this journey, I have many people to thank. First, I would like to express my sincere appreciation to my advisor and mentor, Professor Charles Steinfield who guided and supported me throughout the process. In addition, special thanks are due to my dissertation committee members, Professor Brian Pentland, Pam Whitten, and Dan Kim for their advises and encouragement. I would also like to thank Professor Frank Biocca, Brian Pentland, and Pam Whitten for their guidance support during my years in East Lansing. Finally, I would like to thank colleagues in the INTEnD project: Erik Goodman, John Lloyd, Tim Hind, Ken David, and Ben Pfaff. This project was supported by a grant from the National Science Foundation, IlS-9811568, to Professor Charles Steinfield, Erik Goodman, John Lloyd, Tim Hind, Ken David. TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES .............................................................................. viii LIST OF FIGURES ............................................................................. ix CHAPTER 1. INTRODUCTION ............................................................................... 1 CHAPTER 2. LITERATURE REVIEW AND RESEARCH HYPOTHESES 8 The Rise of Virtual Teams ............................................................ 10 Defining Virtual Teams ................................................... 11 Advantages of Virtual Teams .......................................... 13 Communication Barriers of Virtual Teams .......................... 14 Understanding Awareness ........................................................... 17 Types of Awareness Information ...................................... 20 Awareness and Collaborative Work .................................. 22 Mediated Communication .............................................................. 26 Task Interdependence .................................................................. 27 Task Interdependence, Awareness, and Media Behaviors ................... 28 Media Behavior and Awareness .................................................... 32 Task Interdependence, Media Usage and Awareness ........................ 33 Trust ....................................................................................... 33 Task Interdependence and Trust ................................................... 36 Awareness and Trust .................................................................. 36 Solution Agreement .................................................................... 37 . Task interdependence and Solution Agreement ................................ 38 Awareness and Solution Agreement ............................................... 39 CHAPTER 3. METHODOLOGY ............................................................................. 41 Team composition and Design Projects .......................................... 42 Overview of TeamSCOPE ............................................................ 44 Data Collection ........................................................................... 46 Measurement ............................................................................. 47 CHAPTER 4. RESULTS ........................................................................................ 52 CHAPTER 5. DISCUSSION AND CONLUSION ......................................................... 73 The Effects of Task Interdependence on Media Behaviors .................. 73 vi The Effects of Task Interdependence and Media behavior on Awareness 76 The Effects of Task Interdependence and Awareness on Trust ............ 79 The Effects of Task Interdependence and Awareness on Solution Agreement ............................................................................... 81 Limitations, Implications and Suggestions for Future Research ............ 82 Limitations ..................................................................... 82 Implications for Theories and Future Research ...................... 85 Implications for Groupware Design ...................................... 86 Implications for Practice .................................................... 87 Conclusion ..................................................................... 88 REFERENCES ................................................................................. 98 vii LIST OF TABLES Table 1: Principle Component Analysis: Perceived Awareness ................... 53 Table 2: Principle Component Analysis: Latent variables ........................... 54 Table 3: Descriptive Statistics of Measures Used in the Study .................... 56 Table 4: Bivariate Correlation Matrix .............................. i ........................ 57 Table 5: Bivariate Correlation Matrix (Group Level) .................................. 58 Table 6: Regression Analysis: Predicting Perceived Awareness by Perceived Task Interdependence, Communication Frequency, and TeamSCOPE Usage .................................................................................. 63 Table 7: Regression Analysis: Predicting Perceived Awareness by Perceived Task Interdependence, Communication Frequency, and TeamSCOPE Usage (Group Level) .............................................................. 64 Table 8: Regression Analysis: Predicting Trust by Perceived Awareness and Perceived Task Interdependence .............................................. 69 Table 9. Regression Analysis: Predicting Trust by Perceived Awareness and Perceived Task Interdependence (Group Level) ........................... 70 Table 10. Summary of Hypotheses and Results ...................................... 72 viii LIST OF FIGURES Figure 1. Proposed Research Model ...................................................... 40 Figure 2. TeamSCOPE: Activity Summary Screen ................................... 92 Figure 3. TeamSCOPE: File Activity History ........................................... 93 Figure 4. TeamSCOPE: Message Board ................................................ 94 Figure 5. TeamSCOPE: Login Status, Login Record and Usage Pattern 95 Figure 6. Research Model with Results .................................................. 96 Figure 7. Revised Research Model ....................................................... 97 ix CHAPTER 1 INTRODUCTION Recent development of information and communication technology has enabled organizations to pull resources from all over the world via virtual teams. In virtual teams, people collaborate with each other without physically gathering in the same place as in traditional co-located teams (Lipnack 8 Stamps, 2000). Freeing from geographic limitation, virtual teams offer flexibility and agility that are much needed for businesses to cope with the fierce competition in a global market (Applegate, 1999; Davidow & Malone, 1992; Wigand, Picot, & Reichwald, 1997). However, physical dispersion also brings severe communication and information barriers. One specific challenge that virtual team participants face is awareness deficit - the lack of knowledge about the current state of distant teammates’ work related to the group project. Awareness deficit makes it difficult to coordinate with remote partners and may result in process loss. With the increasing popularity of virtual teams in organizations (Townsend, DeMarie, & Hendrickson, 1998), it is important to further our understanding of awareness in the context of distributed collaboration. The purpose of this study is to investigate the factors that have impacts on awareness as well as the effects of awareness on group outcomes in virtual teams. Specifically, this study aims to examine the influences of task interdependence and media use on perceived awareness level as well as the effects of awareness on trust and solution agreement. Awareness is essential for collaborative work. Ethnographical research on workplace found that collaborators constantly keep each other updated on task- related activities (Richard H.R. Harper, Hughes, 8 Shapiro, 1989; C. C. Heath & Luff, 1991 ). By staying aware of the current state of the project and the activities of coworkers, people are able to adjust and orient their own and their partners” work toward a common goal. In a traditional co-located work setting, it is relatively easy for people to stay updated due to physical proximity. However, when people collaborating from different places, such as in virtual teams, maintaining awareness presents a severe challenge. Traditionally, a project team is brought together in a central location. The close association between physical proximity and awareness can be illustrated by the following quotes from the interview with participants in this study. When asked about how they stay updated with the project progress made by their local teammates, American participants in one of the project teams study answered: G: “It’s not a problem at all. We see each other all the time.” 8: “Yeah, we are practically in the building 20 hours a day.” J: “Yes, us three have a class together, and G, S and T have the same class. Plus, if we don’t meet in the class, we will see each other in the hallway.” S: “Or downstairs." J: “Yes, downstairs, in the workshop.” Meeting in the hallway, workshop, or other shared public areas, was part of their daily routine and workday activities. These gatherings allow team members to easily provide and receive updates regarding the project status. In turn, they can make necessary adjustments and respond to the latest development. Work place studies found that these hallway encounters, coffee room or copy room chats, white board discussions, paper strips, are all commonly used channels that allow people to gather what is happening around them (Richard H.R. Harper et al., 1989; C. C. Heath 8 Luff, 1991). However, these spontaneous connections, informal encounters and peripheral observations, taken for granted in traditional co-located teams, are difficult, if at all possible, when collaborating partners are in different places in a virtual team. Moreover, the locations of virtual team members may span across several time zones. Scheduling meetings can be extremely difficult (Cramton, 1997; Charles Steinfield, Jang, 8 Pfaff. 1999). As a result, unlike traditional co-located teams, virtual teams are short of those genuine channels to stay aware of each others progress or problems. It may further prevent them from responding to those internal issues in a timely fashion. This problem of lack of awareness presents a big challenge of coordination in virtual teams. The issue of awareness in distributed collaboration was first recognized by CSCW, human computer interaction, and groupware research communities. It is perhaps not surprising because, when people begin to work closely from apart, it becomes obvious that they are in danger of awareness deficit. A number of CSCW tools have been designed to address this problem of lack of awareness for distributed teams from different perspectives. Some examples of such tools include Porthole (Dourish 8 Bly, 1992), TeamSCOPE (Charles Steinfleld et al., 1999), and Awareness Monitor (Fussell et al., 1998). Providing various types of awareness information, these tools aim to facilitate distant collaboration without requiring extra communication or cognitive efforts from virtual team participants. However, the current studies in the computer-supported cooperative work and groupware fields are mostly focused on the design issues, such as how to represent different types of awareness information and how to minimize cognitive loads for the users (Schmidt, 2002) The relationships between awareness and other group structural or outcome variables are not investigated. On the other hand, little attention is paid to the issue of awareness in the literature on teams and work groups. It is because, perhaps, previous studies have been focused on co-Iocated work groups. The different ways that people use to maintain awareness are often taken for granted in traditional teams. Mutual awareness is presumed among team members physically working together. However, the setting of virtual teams renders the presumption of mutual awareness no longer feasible. This study aims to inquire about the relationship of awareness and other important team research constructs. Literatures from work group study, group communication, and social psychology help to orient the investigation on the impacts of awareness on group outcomes, especially those deemed important to virtual teams, such as trust and solution agreement. In addition, it costs time and efforts to stay updated particularly in the context of geographically distributed teams. In order to better manage awareness needs, it is helpful to understand the factors that influence the supply and demand of awareness so organizations can anticipate and provide suitable awareness support. Particularly, task interdependence is regarded as a group design parameter that dictates group interaction . The impacts of task interdependence on awareness are studied. Moreover, the role of media in awareness support is also investigated since all communication and information exchanges in virtual teams occur through electronic media. These results help to further our understanding of distributed collaboration and contribute to the management of virtual teams as well as design of collaborative tools. The research questions guiding this study are: - What are the effects of task structure on awareness? - Does media use affect awareness level in virtual teams? - What are the impacts of awareness on trust and solution agreement? The subjects of this study were undergraduate engineering students participating in INTEnD project. INTEnD, standing for International Networked Teams for Engineering Design, was a multi-national research consortium. It was partially funded by National Science Foundation (Charles Steinfield, Goodman, Lloyd, David, 8 Hinds, 1998). This research was part of the INTEnD project and conducted in the fall semester of 2000. Seven virtual engineering design teams participated in the study. Students were recruited from the United States, Russia, and Mexico. These teams were assembled at the beginning of the semester and told to collaboratively complete a 13-week design project. To simulate a real industrial task team, each team worked on a real-world engineering design problem provided by an industry partner. In addition, all teams were also provided with a range of communication tools, including ISDN- and IP-based video conferencing, telephone, fax, email, and TeamSCOPE, which was a web-based groupware developed earlier by INTEnD. Data was collected via survey instrument, user diary, as well as system logs. Multiple regression analysis was applied to the survey data. The results of these analyses suggest that perceived task interdependence and communication frequency had positive effects on perceived level of awareness. Additionally, the effect of communication frequency on perceived awareness was moderated by perceived task interdependence. The results also showed that participants who worked on high interdependent tasks relied more on real-time meetings with distant teammates for awareness. This reliance on synchronous communication channels had positive effects on perceived awareness. Moreover, both perceived task interdependence and awareness were positively associated to trust. Multiple regression analysis showed that perceived awareness was the significant predictor of trust. These results suggest a number of implications for groupware development, virtual team management, and future research. The remaining chapters are organized as following. Chapter 2 provides literature review and research hypotheses. From the systems perspective (McGrath, 1964), awareness can be viewed as a group process variable that links group input and output. Specifically, the concept of awareness and its relationship with other group input and outcome variables will be discussed and hypotheses will be derived. Chapter 3 offers discussion on methodology, including details on the INTEnD project, team composition, data collection and measurements. Results of the study are presented in Chapter 4 and discussed in Chapter 5. In addition, the limitation of the study as well as the implications to future research, virtual team management, and groupware design will also be discussed in Chapter 5. CHAPTER 2 LITERATURE REVIEW AND HYPOTHSES In an attempt to integrate the concept of awareness into the extensive body of literature in work group research, this study applies a systems perspective, which understands group dynamic in terms of input, process, and output. The systems perspective was originated form the early work of McGrath (1964) and Hackman and Morris (1975) in their pursuit of group effectiveness and further developed and adopted by many researchers (Campion, Medsker, 8 Higgs, 1993; Gladstein, 1984; Guzzo 8 Shea, 1992; Hackman, 1987; Pearce 8 Ravlin, 1987; Tannenbaum, Beard, 8 Salas, 1992; Tubbs, 1983). It was regarded as the dominant view of groups historically (Guzzo 8 Shea, 1992). The systems perspective pays special attention to group processes. Group processes describe what happens during group interaction (Hightower 8 Hagmann, 1995). They can be maintenance-oriented that offer social supports (Bales, 1958; Gladstein, 1984), or task-oriented that allow the group to carry out their mission (Campion et al., 1993; Philip 8 Dunphy, 1959). Group processes are posited as linkages between group input, such as group composition, task, and organizational context, and group outcomes, such as task performance, member satisfaction, and other social and psychological output. The basic premises are that group input influences group processes, which, in turn, lead to group outcomes (Tubbs, 1983). Many studies also found that group input, especially task characteristics, moderates the effects of group processes on group outcomes (Gladstein, 1984; Goodman, Ravlin, 8 Argote, 1986; Langfred, 2000). From the systems perspective, awareness is a perfect candidate for a group process variable. Awareness is often referred to as an “understanding of the activities of others” (Dourish 8 Bellotti, 1992)--an essential part of coordination and collaboration. This understanding is important for team coordination because it provides the basis for each member to adjust their action to fit the group. As an integral part of group processes, awareness should be influenced by group input variables and have effects on group outcomes. Specifically, the effects of task interdependence, as a group input variable, on awareness will be examined. Task interdependence dictates the ways team members coordinate with each other. The coordination requirements should produce different demands on awareness. For group outcomes, trust and solution agreement will be discussed. Trust is pivotal in a global virtual team in order to cope with the volatility of the global environment (Sirkka L Jarvenpaa, Knoll, 8 Leidner, 1998). Agreement on team decisions is important for group support in implementing solutions (Castore 8 Murnighan, 1978; Donellon, Gray, 8 Bougon, 1986). Both trust and agreement are challenging for global virtual teams with diverse cultural backgrounds. Awareness as a group process construct is especially important to virtual teams because of the geographic dispersion. In virtual teams, team members have to rely on electronic media for communication and information exchange (Lipnack 8 Stamps, 2000; Townsend et al., 1998). Seeking and presenting project updates or other awareness functions are also conducted via various communication tools and groupware systems. How people use different media for awareness purposes becomes an integral part of group process of virtual teams and will also be investigated. In this chapter, the special characteristics of virtual teams and the communication barriers they bring will be discussed first. The concept of awareness and its impacts on cooperative work will then be reviewed. Following, group input and output variables, task interdependence, trust, and solution agreement, will be introduced. Their relationships with awareness will be discussed in turn and research hypotheses will be proposed. The Rise of Virtual Teams In response to competitive pressures in the global market and to work effectively and efficiently in an international setting, companies have increasingly adopted virtual teams (Boutellier, Gassmann, Macho, 8 Roux;, 1998; Lipnack 8 Stamps, 2000). The fast pace of globalization requires organizations to be more flexible and to utilize their resources more efficiently. Costs of communication for coordinating activities have long been recognized as among the highest in organizations . Organizations traditionally tend to concentrate their personnel in different functional departments at the same location. The rapid development of communication technology, widespread deployment of computer networks, and severe competition in the telecommunication market have greatly reduced the 10 cost of communication and, therefore, enabled companies and firms to structure themselves in different ways (Davidow 8 Malone, 1992; Lucas 8 Baroudi, 1994; Mowshowitz, 1997). One of the popular new organization forms is the virtual team. Defining Virtual Teams A frequently cited definition of a virtual team provided by Lipnack and Stamps (1997) describes virtual teams as “a group of people who interact through interdependent tasks guided by common purpose” and work “across space, time, and organizational boundaries with links strengthened by webs of communication technologies.” Similarly, Benson-Armer and Hsieh (1997) defined a virtual team as a group of geographically dispersed workers brought together to accomplish a specific organizational task using information and communication technologies. Both definitions emphasize the similarity and differences between traditional teams and virtual teams. Virtual teams are similar to traditional teams in the sense that all members work together to achieve a shared goal. However, virtual teams differ from traditional teams in several major aspects. First, members in a virtual team are geographically distributed while traditional teams are gathered in one location. Second, because of spatial separation, virtual team members rely on electronic media for communication. Many other researchers have recognized these two characteristics (Lucas 8 Baroudi, 1994; Maznevski 8 Chudoba, 2000; Sarker, Valacich, 8 Sarker, 2003; Suchan 8 Hayzak, 2001; Townsend et al., 1998). The third major difference, 11 regarded as one of the special properties of virtual teams, is its diversity or boundary-spanning capability. For example, DeSanctis and Monge (1999) defined virtual organization as “a collection of geographically distributed, functionally and/or culturally diverse entities, that are linked by electronic forms of communication and rely on lateral, dynamic relationship for coordination” (p. 693). Suchan and Hayzak (2001) described virtual teams as “collections of geographically dispersed individuals from different functions, specialties, or even organizations” (p. 175). Other research also noted the cultural diversity in virtual teams (Pauleen, 2003; Rutkowski, Vogel, van Genuchten, Bemelmans, 8 Favier, 2002). The flexibility of virtual teams with respect to physical location enables them to include members from different professions, functions, organizations, and countries. As a result, members of virtual teams may bring in the diversity of national cultures, professional trainings, and organizational routines. Suchan and szzak (2001) even suggested that virtual team members have little in common except shared task and interdependence among them. Scholars with different research focuses have noted additional characteristics. For example, virtual teams are thought to be of ad-hoc congregation (Suchan 8 Hayzak, 2001 ). They are called upon for a specific project and dissembled after the mission is accomplished. Sarker, Valacich, and Sarker (2003) defined a virtual team as “a temporary collection of individuals linked primarily through computer and communication technologies working across space and time to complete a specific project” (p.35). Lucas and Baroudi (1994) also characterized virtual teams as temporary groups. In many cases, 12 there is no prior history among virtual team members and no expectation to work together again (S.L. Jarvenpaa 8 Ives, 1994; Lipnack 8 Stamps, 2000). In addition, the membership of a virtual team is fluid, with people joining and leaving during the life of the team according to organizational needs (Lipnack 8 Stamps, 2000) Advantages of Virtual Teams Organizations are driven to search for new structures by the need to leverage knowledge and resources that are inefficiently utilized under traditional structures and also to cope with increasing market competition, product development pressure, and environmental fluctuation (Davidow 8 Malone, 1992; Monge 8 Fulk, 1999; Wigand et al., 1997). Virtual teams, with their agility, flexibility, and cross-boundary capability, provide several distinct advantages attractive to organizations. First, virtual teams free businesses from geographical limitation in assembling project teams. Traditionally, team member selection is confined in a certain area due to the high interaction demands in teamwork. Communication and information technology allow firms to overcome geographic barriers to choose personnel with the best suitable skills and expertise for the task (Bell 8 Kozlowski, 2002; Karolak, 1998; Lipnack 8 Stamps, 2000). This is particularly important in a global market where local expertise holds the key to success (Zirger 8 Maidique, 1990). Second, virtual teams enable companies to access complementary resources, especially in terms of knowledge and expertise. The increasing complexity of product development requires companies to simultaneously work in several knowledge domains (Granstrand, 13 Bohlin, Oskarsson, 8 Sjoberg, 1992; Hagedoom, 1993; Starbuck, 1992). This boundary-spanning capability enables virtual teams to integrate knowledge and expertise from different function areas and organizations. The variety of expertise that members bring in allows organizations to tap a broad array of external information, new knowledge, and expand their capacity for learning and innovating (W. M. Cohen 8 Levinthal, 1990). Third, in a global virtual team, members may all work in different time zones. As a result, the group project can be worked on around the clock. It enables companies to streamline product development process to meet market pressures (Ciborra, 1993). Finally, virtual teams can help organizations cut costs via reduced travel, hiring from low-wage areas, and shortened product development time (Wigand et al., 1997). Communication Barriers of Virtual Teams While virtual teams provide those promising benefits, the special characteristics of virtual teams also bring many challenges. For example, fluid membership may create problems for leadership (Bell 8 Kozlowski, 2002), organization memory, team identity, and cohesion (Kristof, Brown, Henry P. Sims, 8 Smith, 1995). Lack of prior history may contribute to fragile trust among distant teammates (Sirkka L. Jarvenpaa 8 Leidner, 1998). Particularty, virtual teams face many communication and information challenges, including awareness deficits, due to geographical dispersion, cultural diversity, and limited communication modalities. Communication is important to team functions and performance (Simon, 1969). However, team members’ geographical dispersion has negative effects 14 on interaction among teammates. Early study in R8D groups found that the probability of communication between two colleagues dropped with increased physical distance (Allen, 1977). The farther it is between collaborators, the more difficult it becomes to arrange formal meetings and to have spontaneous encounters. Global virtual teams are perfect manifestations of these problems. The difference in cultural backgrounds also adds to difficulties in communication among virtual team members (Pauleen, 2003). Cultural diversity is sourced not only from nationality or ethnicity. Various professional disciplines as well as organizational settings also contribute to it. These different cultures bring in multiple, and possibly incompatible, mindsets, priorities, norms, and values. Working toward mutual understanding can be difficult in this context. Knoll and Jarvenpaa (1998) reported that virtual team participants found it difficult to “predict how others would react and were not always sure what to do about reactions” (p. 10). It typifies the situation when there is no common ground due to cultural diversity. In a review, Bettenhausen (1991) also concluded that the advantages provided by multiple perspectives are often offset by problems in achieving consensus. Without a common ground, it becomes difficult to interpret and respond to messages from distant teammates. Cohen and Mankin (2002) noted similar findings based on their field study of multinational project teams. They also observed that team members learned to consciously avoid slang, colloquialisms, and obscure, culturally based references. It is evident that communication across cultures requires extra cognitive efforts and adds to communication costs for global virtual teams. 15 Adoption of electronic media and software tools is another concern since virtual teams rely on these electronic links. In order to be effective, these networking technologies must be used by a number of people that constitute a critical mass (Markus, 1990). The more users there are the more benefit each user may receive from using the technology. However, to take full advantage of these networking tools, virtual teams face two constraints. First, the technology infrastructures in different locations may not be compatible with each other. Incompatibility will make some of the tools difficult to adopt, or render them to sub-par performance. Both may lead to lower adoption rates and, therefore, reduced overall utility of this tool for the group. In a worse case scenario, it may cause a site to be isolated from the rest of the team due to incompatible systems . System designers have recognized this problem and have begun to set universal accessibility and interoperability as design priorities (Richard Bentley, Horstmann, 8 Trevor, 1997; Charles Steinfield et al., 1999). Second, the diverse cultural contexts of virtual team members may also hinder the adoption and use of new information technologies. For example, in their field study of the adoption of Lotus Notes, Ciborra and Patriatta (1996) found that people at headquarters and remote locations viewed the software as democratic and authoritative respectively. They concluded that pre-existing organizational settings and established work routines have implications on people’s perception and attitude toward groupware tools offered to them. These communication and information deficiencies may harm the teamwork and introduce process loss, including increased time of 16 correspondence (Suchan 8 Hayzak, 2001), slower decision making (N. J. Adler, 1991), and reduced participation (N. J. Adler, 1991). Johnson (1999) noted in his study of partially distributed teams, team members and clients were often tempted to distribute work to the nearby team member instead of the most appropriate person. The higher cost of coordinating and communicating with distant members may drive the team to adopt sub-optimal work arrangements. As a result, the team cannot reach its highest level of performance. One particular coordination issue arising from these communication and information barriers is the problem of awareness deficit. Understanding Awareness The issue of awareness is mostly recognized in the computer-supported cooperative work (CSCW) and groupware research communities. At the early stage of CSCW development, researchers undertook ethnographic studies to further understand work practice in the real world (Richard H.R. Harper et al., 1989; C. C. Heath 8 Luff, 1991). One of the profound contributions of these ethnographic studies is their recognition of how people “seamlessly” align their work with each other via different awareness mechanisms, such as observable actions and shared artifacts. Hughes and his colleagues conducted a series of studies on air traffic controllers (R Bentley et al., 1992; R. H. R. Harper 8 Hughes, 1993; J. Hughes, King, Rodden, 8 Andersen, 1994; John A. Hughes, Randall, 8 Shapiro, 1992; J. A. Hughes, Randall, 8 Shapiro, 1993). They found that air traffic controllers 17 relied on active monitoring of their colleagues' actions and publicly available artifacts, such as the flight strips, to coordinate with their fellow controllers. Health and Luff provided detailed accounts of work practice in the control room of the London Underground (C. Heath 8 Luff, 1992; C. C. Heath 8 Luff, 1991; Luff, Heath, 8 Greatbatch, 1992). They found that the timetable is the primary means by which controllers and signalmen coordinate traffic flow and passenger movement. When making a change on the timetable, controllers will deliberately talk out loudly regarding the change and the rationales for it. This allows the signalmen to be aware of the change without stopping their work on hand. These findings highlighted two important dimensions in discussing awareness. First, awareness as a cognitive state, it refers to having information about a certain set of people, events and objects. For example, Dourish and Bly (1992) referred to awareness as knowing “who is around, what activities are occurring, [and] who is talking with whom.” Similarly, Gutwin, Greenberg and Roseman (1996) defined workspace awareness as the “up-to-the minute knowledge a person holds about another’s interaction with the workspace.” From this cognitive perspective, one central question in designing awareness support in CSCW tools is “what to be aware of.” Researchers have been trying to identify the right types of information to foster desired social exchange or task-related action (Dourish 8 Bly, 1992; Gutwin 8 Greenberg, 1997; Konrad Tollmar, Sandor, 8 Schomer, 1996). A review of five different types of awareness information and correspondent groupware support is provided in the next section. 18 The second dimension, awareness as an action, concerns how people gather and disseminate awareness information. When engaging in collaborative work, people actively monitor activities of their colleagues and changes in shared artifacts. For example, air controllers constantly keep track of the paper strips posted by co-workers containing flight information. At the same time, people also deliberately make their part of the work observable (raising voice, for instance) to help their co-workers stay updated (C. Heath, Svensson, Hindmarsh, Luff, 8 Vom Lehn, 2002). Both gathering and presenting information of work activities is necessary for collaborative partners to maintain awareness. If no one is watching, observable activities will not facilitate awareness. By the same token, if no activity or object is publicly displayed, no awareness information can be attained. This highlights the cooperative nature of awareness conduct. Also it indicates a medium dependence of awareness conduct. It is necessary to have a medium in between, a shared workplace, or a publicly viewable paper strip, so people can obtain and present awareness information. In this study, awareness is considered as a cognitive state. Awareness is defined as possessing “knowledge about the current status and actions of the various components in a collaborative system,” including people, tasks, shared objects, communication media, collaborative software tools, and the work environments (Charles Steinfield et al., 1999). In the context of distributed work, both collecting and delivering awareness information are challenging to collaborative partners. Due to geographical dispersion, virtual team members no longer physically share a workspace. Direct observation is not possible. Sharing 19 artifacts is also impractical unless they are in electronic forms. For virtual teams, all work exchanges will be carried out using electronic media. It is then important to explore the impact of media characteristics on awareness and awareness conduct. A discussion in this regard will be provided in a later section. Types of Awareness Information As discussed above, one central issue of awareness related research is what aspects of the work environment should be represented and monitored in an awareness support system. Five different types of awareness support are found in the literature; they are as follows: 1. Activity awareness Knowledge about projected related activities of other group members is a basic type of awareness information. During real-time collaboration, this may simply mean knowing what actions others are taking at any given moment. Most synchronous collaboration tools thus focus upon on-going activities (eg. Gutwin, Roseman, 8 Greenberg, 1996). However, much group-related activity, such as editing documents, occurs outside synchronous meetings. Asynchronous groupware, such as BSCW (R. Bentley et al., 1997) and most software development tools, often provide awareness of past events by making their log files available. It is especially helpful for group members to be cognizant of any modifications to shared objects such as documents or designs. 2. Availability awareness Many groupware applications monitor the availability of people in order to facilitate informal encounters or social interaction. These include Cruiser (Cool, 20 Fish, Kraut, 8 Lowery, 1992), Portholes (Dourish 8 Bly, 1992), VENUS (Matsuura, Fujino, Okada, 8 Matsushita, 1996), @Work (Konrad Tollmar et al., 1996) and ICQ. Researchers have learned from system trials that in order for social interaction to take place, people need to decide what kind of interaction is appropriate to involve the target party. Therefore, knowing the physical availability of your colleagues is necessary but not sufficient. People also need to know what Tollmar, Sandor and Schomer (1996) called 'social awareness,’ such as whether the target party is busy at the moment. Consequently, he may be unwilling to accept an interaction request despite his presence in the system. In the case of @Work and ICQ, users indicate their physical availability and willingness to interact by selecting from a preset list or inputting text, which also provides information about future availability. 3. Process awareness Process awareness is often found in workflow management systems (eg. (Medina-Mora, Winograd, Flores, 8 Flores, 1992)), where the tasks are usually well defined and represented by a series of sub-tasks. Workflow systems generally assert more control in information flow and in the order in which tasks are completed (Prinz, Rodden, Syri, 8 Trevor, 1996). In order to follow preset procedures, it is useful to provide process awareness, which gives people a sense of where their piece fit into the whole picture, what the next step is, and what needs to be done to move the process along. 21 4. Perspective awareness Anticipation of others' action is important in the coordination of collaborative work (Boland, Schwartz, 8 Tenkasi, 1992; Gutwin et al., 1996). In order to better predict others' actions, people not only need information about others' past actions, but also information on how particular actions emerged. More specifically, this implies giving group member information helpful for making sense of others' actions, such as background on team member’s beliefs and knowledge. This is why Boland et al. (1992) suggested that sharing perspective is required for distributed decision makers. 5. Environmental awareness Environmental awareness focuses on events occurring outside of the immediate workspace that may have implications for group activity. Fussell and colleagues (Fussell et al., 1998), for example, describe a system that tracks important environmental indicators that a business team might use to make decisions. Awareness and Collaborative Work By drawing from the literature of social psychology, social cognition, and CSCW, this section examines how awareness supports collaborative work. It helps us to further understand the impact of awareness and awareness conduct on group processes. Specifically, awareness conduct facilitates collaborative work in three different ways. First, it assists work-related exchange and coordination among members. Second, it motivates people to make positive 22 contribution. Third, it helps to maintain a collaborative mindset. These three aspects will be discussed below. First, awareness supports coordination by triggering interaction among collaborative partners as well as providing a context for individual work. Awareness serves a practical function in enabling social interaction and work exchange. For example, spotting a colleague coming down the hallway allows one to initiate a conversation with him or her. This conversation may involve work-related matters. It may, in turn, lead to further work-related exchanges and adjustments, such as acquiring a new document and rescheduling a meeting. Moreover, by staying aware of what is happening around the workplace, people are able to construct a “view of one another in the daily work environments” (Dourish 8 Bellotti, 1992, p.541 ). This view helps people to see where their individual work fits in the whole picture. As Gutwin, Roseman and Greenberg (1996) pointed out, workspace awareness “provides a context in which to interpret utterances, and allows anticipation of others’ actions” (p.258). The better understanding and anticipation of collaborators” activities will greatly contribute to better coordination. Second, awareness promotes members’ contribution via reducing social loafing or free riding as well as encouraging accountability. Social physiology scholars have long found that people spend less effort when their work is not observed, a phenomena referred to as social loafing (Latane, Williams, 8 Harkins, 1979). Social loafing has been reported in groups carrying out physical tasks (Huddleston, Doody, 8 Ruder, 1985; Swain, 1996; K. D. Williams, Nida, 23 Baca, 8 Latane, 1989) or cognitive tasks (Harkins 8 Petty, 1982; N. Kerr, 1983; N. L. Kerr 8 Bruun, 1983). When the contribution from different members cannot be distinguished, Kerr and Bruun (1981) argued that the anonymity provides the cover necessary for social loafing. However, when individual performance is observable, participants are subject to possible evaluation by co-workers or supervisors. In fact, group members are motivated to observe each other’s work. As Kandel and Lazeer (1992) argued, because a member's reward is often a function of the performance of the whole group, the member has an incentive to monitor other members in the team. The incentive to monitor, together with observable individual actions, creates “evaluation apprehension,” which serves as a performance motivator for team members (Geen, 1991). Studies in work groups found that when the contribution of individual group members can be identified, social loafing is greatly reduced (Price, 1987; Swain, 1996; K. Williams, Harkins, 8 Latane, 1981; K. D. Williams et al., 1989), and the quality of work improves significantly (Szymanski 8 Harkins, 1987; K. Williams et al., 1981) In other words, when individual work is observable, people will be held responsible for their behavior. In this sense, awareness will promote accountability. Literature suggests that people who are held accountable are more likely to perform better and be more attentive to the needs of others than those who are not held accountable for their decisions (Fandt, 1991). In short, as a result of making individual inputs observable, awareness promotes positive and 24 quality contribution to group goals. It also encourages socially amenable behaviors such as being responsible and dependable. Finally, awareness also aids in sustaining a collaborative frame among team members. Frames were defined as “definitions of organizational reality that serve as vehicles for understanding and action” (Gioia, 1986, p.50). From a social cognitive point of view, people act on the basis of their interpretations of the wor1d (Berger 8 Luckmann, 1967; K. E. Weick, 1995). Frames are sets of assumptions, expectations, and knowledge that provide people with references to assign meaning to what happens around them as well as guidelines to take action (Orlikowski 8 Gash, 1994). Lindenberg (1997) applied frame theory in discussing collaboration in work groups. He suggested that in order to have successful teamwork, team members needed to adopt a collaborative frame, which encourages commitments to collective goods and common goals. However, working toward individual gains is always a tempting alternative. Therefore, Lindenberg argued “the salience of cooperative frames will erode unless it is periodically renewed” (Siegwart Lindenberg, 1997, p. 318). Awareness conducts can play a role in confirming the collaborative expectation and, therefore, help to maintain the collaborative frame. As discussed above, publicly perceptible activities and artifacts are important to obtain awareness. To aid and orient the action of fellow collaborators, people may explicitly make their own activities visible, audible, or tractable. This act of serving awareness information indicates an understanding of the task relationship with co-workers as well as co-workers’ information needs resulted 25 from that relationship. By providing the information and/or directing partners’ attention to that information, people at the same time confirm to their fellow group members their commitment to the teamwork. Awareness conduct, In this respect, will serve to maintain the collaborative frame. Mediated Communication Since all communication and information exchanges in virtual teams occur via electronic media, the properties of different media should be examined. Particularly, media richness theory was applied in structuring media characteristics. Media richness theory compares the different media in terms of their capability to convey equivocal information (Daft 8 Lengel, 1984, Trevino, Daft 8 Lengel, 1990) and develops a principle of media choice. From this perspective, media are different in four aspects: speed of feedback, types of sensory channels, degree of social presence, and richness of language carried. With faster feedback, more sensory channels, greater social presence, and richer language, a medium poses higher capacity or is deemed richer. According to these criteria, a richness hierarchy was thus proposed. The level of richness is highest with face-to-face, followed by the telephone, electronic mail, personal documents, such as letters, notes and memos, and impersonal documents, such as fliers, bulletin and report (Trevino, Daft 8 Lengel, 1990). For virtual teams, in general, synchronous channels, such as video conferencing, audio conferencing, text-based chat, should be richer than asynchronous channels, such as emails and groupware. 26 From the viewpoint of media richness theory, a successful communication will be a match between the message and the medium. An equivocal message should be conveyed in a rich media. A mismatch, rich message conveyed in a lean medium or vice versa, will lead to communication failure. In support of this, Daft, Lengel and Trevino (1987) found a strong relationship between managerial performance and manager's capability to communicate effectively. Task Interdependence Interdependence has long been regarded as the essential aspect of a team (Mintzberg, 1979; Salas, Dickinson, Converse, 8 Tannenbaum, 1992; Wall, Kemp, Jackson, 8 Clegg, 1986). It is the “fate of interdependence,” as Lewin argued, that distinguishes a work group from an aggregation of individuals (Lewin, 1951 ). “One person’s experiences, actions and outcomes are linked in some way to the experiences, actions and outcomes of the others in the group” (Brown, 2000, p. 35). Organization research literature suggests many different sources of interdependence in work groups, including the differentiation of roles, the distribution of skills and resources, the manner in which goals are defined and achieved, and the manner in which performance is reviewed and rewarded (for a review, see Wageman, 1995). One of the most important group variables is task interdependence. Particularly, task interdependence is regarded as a dominant factor that dictates how collaborators interact with each other . At the group level, task interdependence is viewed as the extent to which group members are dependent upon one another to perform their individual tasks 27 due to the structural relationship between team members and the nature of the task (Campion et al., 1993; Cummings, 1978; Marvin E. Shaw, 1976; Shea 8 Guzzo, 1987). Task interdependence has been identified to be a key to several group outcome variables, such as group effectiveness (Goodman et al., 1986; Saavedra, Earley, 8 Van Dyne, 1993; Marvin E. Shaw, 1976; Wageman, 1995); job satisfaction (Campion, Pepper, 8 Medsker, 1996); motivation (Hirst, 1988; M. N Kiggundu, 1983; Wong 8 Campion, 1991); and group norm (Wageman, 1995). Most importantly, for the current study, task interdependence is one of the group- design parameters that have predictive power on group communication and information exchange (Gresov, 1989; Hackman 8 Morris, 1975; Kelly 8 McGrath, 1985; Maznevski 8 Chudoba, 2000; Straus 8 McGrath, 1994; A. Van de Ven et al., 1976; Vegt, Emans, 8 Vliert, 1999). Task Interdependence, Awareness, and Media Behavior Thompson’s (1967) classification of task interdependence provides an account on how it dictates group interaction patterns. He distinguished three different levels of task interdependence, including pooled, sequential, and reciprocal interdependence, among work units. Each type of task interdependence sets a different communication requirement among work units. In a pooled-interdependence arrangement, work outputs are turned in independently and aggregated as the group output. There is no need of work interaction between participants. In a sequential-interdependence arrangement, the output of one member constitutes the input of another member. They are 28 required to connect with one another in a predetermined order. Finally, when working on a reciprocal-interdependent task, co-workers are required to exchange their work output with each other in order to integrate into the group output—when task structure moves, from pooled and sequential, to reciprocal, the level of interdependence increases. With increased interdependence, collaborating parties rely more on each other in order to do their own job. As a result, the mutual dependence among collaborators requires tighter coordination and, in turn, posts higher demand for awareness information. The more collaborative partners depend on each other, the more they need to stay updated in order to coordinate their works. Task interdependence is, therefore, a motivator that prompts team members to provide and acquire awareness information. Accordingly, the following hypothesis is proposed: H1: Higher level of perceived task interdependence will lead to higher level of perceived awareness. In addition, the mutual dependency embedded in the task structure can be seen as a source of uncertainty and, accordingly, a source of information demand (Galbraith, 1973; A. H. Van de Ven, A. L. Delbecq, 8 R. J. Koenig, 1976). The greater the degree of task interdependence, the greater the problem- solving demands and the greater information processing requirements becomes (March 8 Simon, 1958). 29 Both lead to more frequent and flexible interaction between collaborators. In support of this, Staples 8 Jarvenpaa (2000) found that, in an organizational setting, the use of electronic media increased when people perceived a higher level of task interdependence with one another. In fact, “team,” with all members gathering in a single location traditionally for easy connection, is regarded as a special work arrangement to carry out highly interdependent tasks (A. Van de Ven et al., 1976). Following the literature, higher level of task interdependence will lead to more frequent information exchange among team members. In the case of virtual teams with groupware support, more information exchange should translate to higher communication frequency and more usage of the groupware. Specifically, TeamSCOPE, a web-based groupware, were provided to participating teams in this study. Details on TeamSCOPE will be provided in the next chapter. Thus, the following hypothesis is proposed: H2a: Higher level of perceived task interdependence will lead to higher communication frequency. H2b: Higher level of perceived task interdependence will lead to more TeamSCOPE usage. In addition, high interdependent task demands constant mutual adjustment to deal with the uncertainty of a job. Based on media richness theory, rich media, that conveys more social cues and allows immediate 3O feedback, is better suited for tasks with high uncertainty. Strauss and McGrath (1994) found that, when working on a highly interdependent task, the group using rich media had higher productivity. For awareness purpose, team members perceiving high task interdependence should prefer using rich media, such as synchronous channels, so that they can better interpret the awareness signals as well as offer timely responses. Plus, coordinating team members to set up a synchronous connection is never easy. It is especially true for global virtual teams, since they have only a limited time window, if any, to work together simultaneously. As a result, synchronous work sessions become a luxury to global virtual teams. Without task requirement, team members perceiving low task interdependence will be less motivated to use rich media for awareness or other information purpose. In other words, when perceived task interdependence decreases, participants will be more likely to rely on lean media to stay updated. However, the reliance on lean media should be not exclusive. As Adler (1991) pointed out that the use of more interactive channel does not precludes simultaneous use of less interactive channels. Due to increased awareness demand, participants working with high interdependent tasks should also utilize lean media for awareness purpose. Accordingly, the following hypotheses are proposed: H3a: Higher level of perceived task interdependence will lead to greater perceived reliance on synchronous communication channel for awareness purpose. 31 H3b: Perceived task interdependence will have no effect on the extent to which asynchronous communication media is relied upon for awareness purpose. Media Behavior and Awareness Since all interaction among members is carried through electronic media in virtual teams, awareness information seeking and presenting have to be conducted via the networking technology. The more frequent participants use these media, the more opportunities there are for them to gather awareness information. Thus, the following hypothesis is proposed. H4a: Higher communication frequency will lead to higher level of perceived awareness. H4b: More TeamSCOPE usage will lead to higher level of perceived awareness. As discussed above, rich media afford more social cues, multiple sensory channels, and instant feedback capability. They will be able to better accommodate the awareness needs of virtual team members. Thus, the following hypothesis is proposed: H5: Greater reliance on synchronous communication channel for awareness purpose will lead to higher level of perceived awareness. 32 Task Interdependence, Media Usage and Awareness As discussed above, both task interdependence and media usage are proposed to have positive effects on the perceived level of awareness (H1, H3). However, the strength of the effects may be different under different circumstances. The degree of task interdependence dictates the degree of awareness demand. When participants perceive a higher level of task interdependence, their needs for awareness information increase. Therefore, more communication is required to avoid the problem of awareness deficit. By the same token, group members who perceive a lower level of task interdependence require less communication to saturate their awareness needs. In summary, task interdependence will moderate the effects of communication frequency and groupware use on perceived awareness level. H6a: The effect of communication frequency on level of awareness will be weaker for people who perceive higher degree of task interdependence. H6b: The effect of groupware usage on level of awareness will be weaker for people who perceive higher degree of task interdependence. Trust Trust is an important factor that influences group processes and group performance. Literature showed that researchers found trust as a predictor or moderator for group performance, organizational citizenship behaviors, effort 33 level, job satisfaction, as well as interpersonal relationship within a group (Alge, Wiethoff, 8 Klein, 2003; Mayer, Davis, 8 Schoorman, 1995; Morris, Marshall, 8 Rainer, 2002). Particularly, trust is regarded as an essential ingredient for cooperation (Blau, 1964; Coleman, 1988; Hwang 8 Burgers, 1997; Putnam, 2000; Zucker, 1986). The importance of trust in collaborations can be understood via the concept of risk. For example, Currall and Judge (1995) defined trust as “an individual’s behavioral reliance on another person under a condition of risk” (p. 153). This risk is the likelihood of being exploited by a partner. Without trust, a person is tempted to act opportunisticallyucollect the short-term gains instead of long-term gains and greater goods (Hwang 8 Burgers, 1997). This risk is also conceived as part of the transaction cost of exchange, and, without trust, it may make the transaction too costly to be carried out (Bromiley 8 Cummings, 1995). On the other hand, when engaging in a trusting relationship, parties are more willing to overlook the risk of being taken advantage of and act for collective gains. From this perspective, trust is particularly important to collaboration. As Hwang and Burgers (1997) argued, trust will facilitate cooperation by reducing the overall opportunistic behaviors and, therefore, the risk of being exploited, as well as by keeping a trustworthy partner in the relationship. Empirical research confirmed that mutual trust among participants is one of the prerequisites for collaboration to happen in a team (Druskat 8 Wolff, 2001). It has also been suggested that the development of interpersonal trust has a cognitive foundation (Lewis 8 Weigert, 1985; McAllister, 1995). Researchers 34 from different fields agree that trust develops through repeated social interactions that enable people to update their information about other’s trustworthiness (Gabarro, 1978; Lewicki 8 Bunker, 1996; Mayer et al., 1995; McAllister, 1995; Rempel, Holmes, 8 Zanna, 1985). As Lewis and Wiegert (1985) argued, our decision to trust others is based on “what we take to be ‘good reasons,’ constituting evidence of trustworthiness” (p.970). This evidence or knowledge about others enables us to develop the schemes upon which we draw our prediction on others’ behavior (Sarker et al., 2003), and it is our knowledge about others’ competence (Butler, 1991), reliability (Johnson-George 8 Swap, 1982), and dependability (Rempel et al., 1985) that forms the basis of trust. Zucker (1986) also argued that, in a trusting relationship, parties expect their partners to act in a reliable and dependable fashion. In a team context, Sarker, Valecich, and Sarker (2003) suggested that “trust develops because team members are able to gain knowledge about the other collaborators through increased familiarity, and thus, able to confidently predict their behaviors (Coutu, 1998). This prediction of behaviors is possible primarily through the exchange of task- related information in a consistent and reliable manner” (p.38). One study also found that informal monitoring assists trust building among international partners (Aulakh, Kotabe, 8 Sahay, 1996). In short, to develop and maintain trust, people need to be able to examine if their partners meet the expectation of reliability and dependability. This is especially important in the context of virtual team, where initial trust can be very fragile (Sirkka L. Jarvenpaa 8 Leidner, 1998). 35 Task Interdependence and Trust Mutual dependence embedded in group work presumes a degree of risk. The higher task interdependence, the more collaborative partners relied on each other to complete the job. Therefore, high interdependence makes it increasingly dangerous for partners to engage in opportunistic behavior or negative tactics because all parties have much to lose (Kumar, Scheer, 8 Steenkamp, 1995). Accordingly, people working in high interdependent task are more motivated to build trust. Thus, the following hypothesis is proposed: H7: Higher level of perceived task interdependence will lead to higher level of trust. Awareness and Trust Awareness and awareness conduct may facilitate trust. Awareness facilitates trust development in two aspects. First, it allows partners to confirm expectations and predictions on teammates’ behavior. As discussed above, team members need to acquire knowledge about their collaborators’ action for trust building and maintenance. Awareness mechanisms provide the necessary support that allows team members to confirm and refine their expectations on their distant partners. Second, awareness promotes trusting behaviors. As discussed above, from a social psychology perspective, mutual awareness may dissuade opportunistic conducts, and therefore, reduce social loafing. Moreover, 36 by making individual contributions visible, awareness encourages dependable and reliable conduct of group members, which will lead to positive contribution to the group project. Thus, the following hypothesis is proposed: H8: Higher level of perceived awareness will lead to higher level of trust. Solution Agreement Members’ agreement on group decision and design solution is a desired group outcome because it affects members’ support to implement the solution (Castore 8 Murnighan, 1978). It is especially important for a design project, since selecting from design alternatives is often only half of the job. Project teams need to implement their design solution, in the form of a prototype, for instance, after they make the design choice. Prior research suggests that both task types and group interaction have implications on group consensus (Argote 8 McGrath, 1993; Hightower 8 Hagmann, 1995; Straus, 1999). Particularly, they will affect how information exchange as well as normative influences among members occurs in the process of reaching a consensus (Siegel, Dubrovsky, Kiesler, 8 McGuire, 1986). Information and opinion exchange is essential in discussion and negotiation toward an agreement among group members (Feldman, 1989; K. Weick, 1979). Particularly, literature suggests that task-oriented communication will enhance group consensus (Kahai 8 Cooper, 1999; McGrath 8 Hollingshead, 1993). In support of that, Lam and Schaubroeck (2000) found that groups reach 37 higher consensus on group decision when group members share their task- related information. In a study on groups supported by audio/video-conferencing systems, Yoo and Alavi (2001) also found that task participation has positive relationship with a degree of consensus among members of established groups. Since the nature of a group’s task may impose different requirements on group interaction (Hackman 8 Morris, 1975; Kelly 8 McGrath, 1985), it will have impacts on group consensus. In addition, participants’ conformation to group goals also contributes to consensus building. As Souder (1975) pointed out, the recognition and commitment of a collective goal is one of the prerequisites for reaching a consensus in an organizational setting. Normative arguments based on shared group goals can help to win acceptance for a particular design alternative. The more members are willing to conform to the group, the more effective the normative influences are, and the more likely the group will reach their decision in agreement. Task interdependence and Solution Agreement Task interdependence, as discussed above, requires a higher degree of coordination among members and promotes more frequent task-related communication. Accordingly, Straus (1999) suggests that the degree of task interdependence will be positively associated with the amount of group communication that reflects consensus building and coordination processes in 38 groups. The relationship between task interdependence and solution agreement is proposed as below: H9: Higher level of perceived task interdependence will lead to higher level of perceived solution agreement. Awareness and Solution Agreement Awareness will help as groups reach an agreement by facilitating the information flow as well as establishing collaborative norms. As discussed above, activity and availability awareness information serve to trigger task—related interaction and, therefore, increase task-related information exchange. In addition, awareness and awareness conducts help groups to maintain a collaborative norm and promote group conformable behaviors. Both should make positive contribution to group consensus building. Accordingly, the following hypothesis is proposed: H10: Higher level of perceived awareness will lead to higher level of perceived solution agreement. The proposed research model is presented in Figure 1. 39 Figure 1. Proposed Research Model Perceived Task Interdependence H7 (+) _ H8 (+) Trust H6a (-) H2a (+) CF H4a (+) H6b (-) Perceived H2b (+) TU r Awareness H4b (+) H3a (+) RD H5 (+) H1 (+) H10 (+) Perceived Solution H9 (+) Agreement CF: Communication frequency TU: TeamSCOPE usage RD: Perceived reliance on distant meetings for awareness purposes 40 CHAPTER 3 METHODOLOGY This research is based on the International Networked Teams for Engineering Design (INTEnD) project. INTEnD was initiated by Michigan State University in fall, 1997 and joined by several universities from the Netherlands, Russia, China, Spain, and Mexico. The project was aimed to study how to best support globally distributed engineering design teams. The primary research objective was to understand the communication and coordination processes in virtual teams. One specific research interest, based on CSCW literature and prior pilot studies, was the provision and consumption of awareness information in globally distributed teams. To this end, a web-based collaborative system called TeamSCOPE has been designed with an explicit attempt to fulfill the awareness needs in virtual teams. Research results concerning the media use patterns, activity awareness, TeamSCOPE evaluation and methodology issues studying virtual teams have been reported in conferences and journals (see Huysman et al., forthcoming; Jang, Steinfield, 8 Pfaff. 2000, 2002; C. Steinfield et al., 2001; Charles Steinfield et al., 1999). TeamSCOPE was also provided to the participants of this study. A brief overview of features of TeamSCOPE will be provided in a later section. The author was a research assistant to the INTEnD project and was involved in all phases of the research including preparing data collection 41 instruments, administering questionnaires, conducting field observation and interviews, and authoring research papers. One of continuous interests of the author is to fuse the group research literature with the study of distributed collaboration. Particularly, the author is interested in the effects of task interdependence, which, as discussed above, has great impacts on group processes. Through interviews, field observation, and informal contacts with virtual teams in the INTEnD project, the author found that although all teams working on engineering design projects, there were different levels of interdependence among team members which seemed to contribute to the differences in their communication and coordination behaviors. The author then proposed to include task interdependence, which is not within the original scope of the INTEnD investigation, into the final phase of the project and formed the basis of this dissertation. Team composition and Design Projects Virtual team formation and engineering design project recruitment were managed by engineering faculty members in the INTEnD project. At each of the schools, one or more engineering faculty members recruited students to work on virtual design teams. Throughout the project, engineering faculty members worked with the students intensively as design project supervisors, providing advice, and finally, evaluated their work. Participants recruited for this study were all upper-level undergraduate students. They were told they would be working on an international student team. Team members worked with each 42 other in English, and students had to possess English language skills to work on the projects. To simulate a professional environment of engineering design teams, faculty members recruited industrial partners for real design projects. All students recruited consented to participate in this study voluntarily. Industry partners had to agree to be the "client" for the student team and provide contact persons to interact with the student teams. This meant being available to meet with team members to describe and discuss their needs and give feedback on group output. In the fall semester of 2000, 7 distributed engineering student design teams were recruited to participate in this study. Students were from Michigan State University, St. Petersburg State Technical University in Russia, Texas A8M University, and three campuses of the Monterrey Institute of Technology in Mexico. Each team was composed of 6 to 10 members, and these members are from Michigan State University and one other location. They were assembled at the beginning of the semester and told to collaboratively complete a design project sponsored by an industry partner. There were 53 total participants. The design projects lasted about 13 weeks. The projects included design of automotive air conditioning system refinement, stair—climbing wheelchair, small animal intensive care unit, end effector storage unit, mechatronics laboratory demonstration system, CAD modeling of automotive structures, and automotive thermo electric units. 43 Each team was offered with a set of communication tools, including ISDN- based video conferencing system (VTEL), Microsoft Netmeeting, telephone, fax, a project-specific email account, as well as TeamSCOPE. Training sessions and instructional materials for the use of video conferencing systems and TeamSCOPE were provided at the beginning of the design projects. Participants were also free to use other communication tools, such as ICQ. Overview of TeamSCOPE TeamSCOPE was designed to meet the needs of a common information repository and awareness support for distributed collaborative work (Charles Steinfield et al., 1999). It features a shared file space, calendar, message board, and several awareness related functions. Shared file space was deemed the most useful feature by the past users. It allows users to share and manage their project-related electronic materials. To increase the accessibility and interoperatability, TeamSCOPE was completely web-based. All functions and features of TeamSCOPE can be accessed through standard web browsers. One of the main goals of TeamSCOPE was to provide awareness support for virtual project teams. To this end, all activities occurred in the shared file space and other functional areas were recorded and posted into a database. These records of activities are then presented in various ways for users to conveniently acquire awareness information. The awareness functions include activity summary, file activity history, viewing records, login status, and login 44 history. For the purpose of this study, the rest of this section focuses on introducing the awareness supports provided by TeamSCOPE. The default screen after login is a summary of all recent activities, including file uploads, comments and messages posted, calendar entries. User's accesses to all entries are also recorded and displayed. Objects are provided as links, so that team members can go immediately to them once they are aware that something has happened (Figure 2). To avoid information overload, TeamSCOPE also allows users to set filters for the activity summary screen. They can request TeamSCOPE to show only information related to certain files or users. Insert Figure 2 about here. In addition, all file related activities were tracked and presented to users when they access a specific file. These activities include file upload, download, as well as posting and reading comments on the specific file (Figure 3). Similarly, for each calendar and message board entry, TeamSCOPE provides a list of users who has read it (Figure 4). Insert Figure 3 and 4 about here. Moreover, all activity awareness information collected by TeamSCOPE is also available via email. Users can specify how frequent they want to receive the awareness notification email. They can also apply filters to focus on activities related to specific teammates or files. Finally, at the beginning of every page in TeamSCOPE, there is a notification of who else on the team also happens to be logged in within the past 45 5 minutes. It, to some extend, indicates the availability of teammates. Users may choose to initiate a chat session or use other synchronous tools for contact. The login record of each team member was also complied to construct a user information page. It allows team members to review each other's login history over a specified time period. It also offering graphical presentation of what times of day others have connected in the specified time period. This is displayed in the querying users local time to help them predict when they might find their teammates online (Figure 5). Insert Figure 5 about here. Data Collection Three sets of data have been collected. First, participants submitted a weekly communication log. They were asked to report all communication events with their distant partners every week. For each event, they also reported all the media they used during the event. The usage of TeamSCOPE was not included weekly communication reports from participants. Instead, it was based on its system record of all page requests made to the system. Second, a questionnaire was administrated at the end of the project to measure participants’ perception on task interdependence, awareness channel reliance, awareness, trust, as well as solution agreement. Finally, semi-structured interviews and observations of cross-location team meetings helped to provide accounts of the nature of group processes. 46 Measurement Task interdependence Task interdependence is conceived as the extent to which group members are dependent upon one another to perform their individual tasks due to the structural relationship between team members and the nature of the task (Campion et al., 1993; Cummings, 1978; Marvin E. Shaw, 1976; Shea 8 Guzzo, 1987). Following previous studies (Campion et al., 1993; Campion et al., 1996; M. N Kiggundu, 1983; Liden, Wayne, 8 Bradway, 1997; Van der Vegt, Emans, 8 Vande Vliert, 1999; Wageman, 1995), task interdependence was measured as team members’ perception about the structure of tasks. Nine 5-point items were adapted from the above-cited literature to form the scale of task interdependence. IDP1 My local group's performance depends on receiving information and advice from our distant teammates. IDP2 My distant teammates' performance depends on receiving information and advice from my local group. IDP3 My local group depends on our distant teammates' work that is needed before we can do our part of the project. IDP4 My distant teammates depend on my local group's work that is needed before they can do their part of the project. IDP5 My local group depends on our distant teammates in order to be able to do our job well. IDP6 My distant teammates depend on my local group in order to be able to do their job well. IDP7 The outcome of our project is strongly affected by our distant teammates' performance. IDP8 Our distant teammates have certain knowledge or skill that is critical to our project. IPDQ* My local group can complete the project well enough without contribution from our distant teammates. 5-point Likert scale: 1=Strong|y disagree; 5=Strongly Agree 47 Awareness channel reliance Prior studies of the INTEnD project found that three communication channels were used commonly by virtual design teams. These channels include emails, TeamSCOPE activity summary, and conversation with distant teammates in meetings. For each of the above channels, participants were asked to rate the extent to which, in a five-point Likert scale (5 = very much, 1 = not at all), they relied on it to acquire project-related awareness information. To what extend do you rely on the following channels to learn that your distant teammates did work related to the project? RE Emails from my teammates RT TeamSCOPE activity summary RD Conversation with my distant teammates in our meeting 5-point Likert scale: 1 = Not at all; 5 = Very much Trust Following (Sirkka L. Jarvenpaa 8 Leidner, 1998), an 8-item scale on trust was adopted from (Schoorman, Mayer, 8 Davis, 1996) to assess ability, benevolence, and integrity. Participants were asked about their perceptions of these attributes of their distant partners. T1 Members of my work group show a great deal of integrity. T2 I can rely on those with whom I work in this group. T3 Overall, the people in my group are very trustworthy. T4 We are usually considerate of one another’s feelings in this work group. T5 The people in my group are friendly. T6 There is no "team spirit" in my group. T7 There is a noticeable lack of confidence among those with whom I work. T8 We have confidence in one another in this group. 48 Awareness Awareness is defined as possessing knowledge about the current status and actions of the various components in a collaborative system. Nine 5-point items were developed by prior INTEnD studies to measure the different types of awareness. These nine items were also applied in this study. AW1 I usually had a good idea of what my distant teammates were working on. AW2 I felt that my distant team members had a good idea of what we were working on here AW3 I had a good idea of the typical times of the day when my distant teammates were working on the project AW4 I felt that my distant team members had a good idea of the times of the day when I was working on the project AW5 I had enough information to know when I could generally contact my distant teammates for a phone call or computer chat without scheduling it ahead of time. AW6* During the project, I often felt that I did not know what my distant teammates were working on AW7 I always knew when the task I was working on needed to be finished AW8 I felt like I knew my distant teammates pretty well AW9* I wish I knew more about my distant teammates backgrounds 5-point Likert scale: 1 = Not at all; 5 = Very much * Reverse coded. 49 Solution Agreement Based on (Maier, 1963; Tubbs, 1983), two 5-point items were developed to measured how group members evaluated the group final product. Each participant was asked to rate the extent to which he or she agreed with the solution and the extent to which he or she thought the solution addressed the problem. A1 How much did you agree with the group's solution? A2 How well did you feel the group's solution addressed the problem? Hypotheses testing strategy To test the relationships that were proposed in the hypotheses, the following procedures are applied. First, in order to decrease the likelihood of multicollinearity in equations having interaction terms, all the data are centered around variable means -- replacing value by deviations from the means (J. Cohen 8 Cohen, 1983). This procedure was also suggested for better interpreting the coefficient of the interaction terms (Jaccard 8 Turrisi, 2003). For main effect hypotheses, regression analysis is applied. To test moderator hypotheses, two-step regression models is applied. In step one, the dependent variable is regressed on the independent variables for main effects. In step two, the product of the independent variables is added into the model as the interaction term. The dependent variable was regressed on the independent variables and the interaction term. The difference in the multiple squared 50 correlation coefficients (Rz’s) between the main effect model and the interaction effect model were calculated to compare the fit of each model. In addition, all hypotheses were also tested at the group level for exploratory purpose. Group average of individual data were used for group level analysis. Same statistic procedures were applied. 51 CHAPTER 4 RESULTS Factor Structure and Scale Reliability All scales used in this study undenivent the following procedures to determine their dimensionality and ensure internal consistency. First, confirmatory factor analysis was conducted in each scale to test internal consistency. Only items with .50 loading or higher are retained. In addition, each item is examined for face validity based on existing theory and logical judgment. Furthermore, a coefficient alpha is calculated to test internal consistency. Due to the small sample size and exploratory nature of this study, .70 was used for the scale reliability criteria. Finally, items that remained after the internal consistency test were appropriately recoded if necessary. A single composite measure for each variable was then calculated using the average score of the remaining items. Summated scales were created to provide a means of overcoming to some extent the measurement error inherent in all measured variables to accurately provide information. The averaged response to a set of related variables is used in order to reduce the measurement error that might occur in a single question. 52 Task Interdependence Nine items were adopted from literature to measure participants’ perception of task interdependence. Principle component analysis with Varimax rotation was conducted and three components were extracted. After examining face validity and internal reliability, item IDP7, IDP8, IPD9 was selected to form the scale of task interdependence. Awareness Nine items related to awareness were included in the questionnaire. Principle component analysis with Varimax rotation was conducted and three components were extracted (see Table 1). Component 1 and 2 corresponded to availability and activity awareness respectively. Both scales met the reliability criteria. Items loaded in component 3 were not conceptually coherent and was dropped from further analysis. Due to the focus of this study, only activity awareness was considered in the following hypothesis testing. Table 1. Principle Component Analysis: Perceived Awareness Component 1 2 3 AW3 .919 AW4 .920 AW5 .497 AW6 .792 AW1 .739 AW8 .699 AW2 .519 AW9 .881 AW7 .719 Rotation Method: Varimax with Kaiser Normalization. 53 Trust Scale Confirmatory factor analysis was conducted on the trust measurement adopted from Schoorman, Mayer, and Davis (1996). Six items remained to form the trust scale. The coefficient alpha for trust scale is .87. Finally, a principle component analysis performed on all selected items for each scale showed that all items were loaded highest in their respective scale (see Table 2). Table 2. Principle Component Analysis: Latent variables Component Trust Perceived Perceived task Awareness interdependence T1 .634 T2 .607 T3 .825 T4 .718 T5 .663 T8 .838 AW1 .682 AW2 .593 AW6 .785 AW8 .555 IDP7 .562 IDP8 .743 IDP9 .882 Rotation Method: Varimax with Kaiser Normalization. Communication Frequency Communication frequency was the average number of weekly communication incidents with distant partners. It was based on participants’ weekly communication log. All communication events with distant teammates 54 were counted, including video meetings, audio conferences, text-based chat sessions, emails and faxes. The average number of communication events per week was calculated as the communication frequency and used for following analyses. TeamSCOPE Usage TeamSCOPE usage was based on its system log of user page requests. The average number of page requests per week was calculated to form the measurement of TeamSCOPE usage. 50 of the 53 participants filled out the questionnaire. See Table 3 for descriptive statistics of measures used in the study. The bivariate correlation matrix for studies variables was presented in Table 4 (individual level) and Table 5 (group level). 55 Table 3. Descriptive Statistics of Measures Used in the Study Number of Standard items Coefficient Variables Mean Deviation included Alpha Perceived task interdependence 3.27 1 .10 3 .73 Distant dependence 3.69 1.06 3 .81 Awareness 3.53 0.85 4 .74 Trust 4.12 0.72 6 .87 Solution agreement 4.42 0.59 2 .71 Communication Frequency 1.46 1.16 TeamSCOPE Usage 16.96 15.58 56 Table 4. Bivariate Correlation Matrix TI CF TU RD RE RT AW TR Perceived Task Interdependence (TD Communication Frequency (CF) .283' TeamSCOPE Usage (TU) .139 .313' Perceived Reliance on Distant Meeting for Awareness (RD) .566"' .268T -.005 Perceived Reliance on Emails for Awareness (RE) 4343' 4250* -.104 -.109 Perceived Reliance on TeamSCOPE for Awareness (RT) .216 -.028 .248f .156 -.150 Perceived Awareness (AW) 7" .473 .299' -.164 .661"‘ -.196 .015 Perceived Trust (TR) .454"T* .141 .020 .540"' 4294' .087 .665"' Perceived Solution Agreement (SA) .097 .193 .027 .284' -.142 -.059 .055 .047 N=50 * significant at .05 level, ** significant at .01 level, **" significant at .001 level I Moderately significant at .10 level 57 Table 5. Bivariate Correlation Matrix (Group Level) Tl CF TU RD RE RT AW TR Perceived Task Interdependence (TD Communication Frfluency (CPL .637 TeamSCOPE Usage (TU) .280 .487 Perceived Reliance on Distant Meeting for Awareness (RD) .827' .718T .064 Perceived Reliance on Emails for Awareness (RE) a683f a806' -.251 -.619 Perceived Reliance on TeamSCOPE for Awareness ET) .223 .429 .679T .071 -.522 Perceived Awareness (AW) .734T .620 -.071 .894" -.667 -.083 Perceived Trust (TR) .828' .529 -.007 .898" -.632 .008 .960" Perceived Solution Agreement (SA) .495 .207 .032 .221 -.133 -.024 -.046 .020 N=7 * significant at .05 level, ** significant at .01 level, *** significant at .001 level I Moderately significant at .10 level 58 The Effect of Task Interdependence on Communication Frequency and TeamSCOPE Usage H2a: Higher level of perceived task interdependence will lead to higher communication frequency. H2b: Higher level of perceived task interdependence will lead to more TeamSCOPE usage. Results showed that the effect of perceived task interdependence on communication frequency was positive and significant (r = .283, p = .046). H2a was supported. Participants who perceived higher degree of task interdependence reported more communication events. The usage of TeamSCOPE was positively associated with perceived task interdependence; however, their relationship was not statistically significant (r = .139, p = .363). H2b was not supported. Group level analysis did not find statistically significant results, although both communication frequency and TeamSCOPE usage were positively related to task interdependence. The Effects of Perceived Task Interdependence, Communication Frequency and TeamSCOPE Usage on Perceived Awareness H1: Higher level of perceived task interdependence will lead to higher level of perceived awareness. H4a: Higher communication frequency will lead to higher level of perceived awareness. 59 H4b: More TeamSCOPE usage will lead to higher level of perceived awareness. As hypothesized, perceived task interdependence (r = .473, p = .001) was a significant predictor of perceived awareness. H1 was supported. In addition, this hypothesis was moderately supported at the group level (r = .734, p = .060). Results also showed that communication frequency has a significant and positive effect on perceived awareness (r = .299, p = .035). The relationship between TeamSCOPE usage and perceived awareness was not significant (r = - .164, p = .281 ). H4a was supported, but H4b was not. H6a: The effect of communication frequency on perceived awareness will be weaker for people who perceive higher degree of task interdependence. H6b: The effect of TeamSCOPE usage on perceived awareness will be weaker for people who perceive higher degree of task interdependence. To test hypotheses 6a and 6b, a series of multiple regression analysis were conducted (see Table 6). Model 1 and 2 tested the main effects and interaction effects of perceived task interdependence and communication frequency on perceived awareness. Model 3 and 4 assessed the main effects and interaction effects of perceived task interdependence and TeamSCOPE usage on perceived awareness. Finally, perceived awareness level was regressed on task interdependence, communication frequency and TeamSCOPE usage for main effects (Table 6, Model 5) and first-order interaction effects (Table 6, Model 6). Similar procedures, excepting Model 6, were also applied to the group level data, due to limited sample size (Table 7). 6O Awareness level was first regressed on perceived task interdependence and communication frequency (Table 6, Model 1 and 2). The effect of perceived task interdependence on perceived awareness was positive and significant on both the main effect model ([3 = .422, p = .002) and interaction model (B = .313, p = .023). The effect of communication frequency was significant in the interaction model (B = .461, p = .010). The interaction effect (T l x CF) was also significant (8 = -.396, p = .021 ). The negative coefficient indicated that, when perceived task interdependence increased, the effect of communication frequency on perceived awareness decreased. H6a was supported in this model. The interaction model also explained more variance in perceived awareness level than the main effect model (AR2 = .083). An F test showed that this difference in R2 was significant. Perceived level of awareness was then regressed on perceived task interdependence and TeamSCOPE usage (Table 6, Model 3 and 4). Perceived task interdependence was also found to be positively and significantly associated to perceived awareness in both the main effect model (B = .505, p <.001) and the interaction model (B = .510, p = .001). The effect of TeamSCOPE usage was moderately significant in both the main effect model (B = -.235, p =.084) and the interaction model (B = -.239, p =.087) in the main effect model. However, the negative coefficient of TeamSCOPE usage was in contradiction with H4b. The interaction term (Tl x TU) was not significant. The explaining power of the 61 interaction model was almost identical to that of the main effect model (AR2 = .001). H6b was not supported. Finally, awareness level was regressed on perceived task interdependence, communication frequency and TeamSCOPE usage (Table 6, Model 5 and 6). To determine if the effects of communication frequency and TeamSCOPE usage were additive, an interaction term (CF x TU) was added in the interaction model. Since no second-order moderator effect was proposed, three-way interaction was not included in the interaction model. On both main effect and interaction model, perceived task interdependence and communication frequency were positively contributed to the perceived awareness, while TeamSCOPE usage was negatively contributed to perceived awareness. The interaction between perceived task interdependence and communication frequency was again significant and negative as proposed in H6a. The other interaction terms were not statistically significant. Multiple regress analyses at the group level found similar results (Table 7). The standardized coefficients in all main effect and interaction models maintained the same direction as in the individual level analyses, excepting the one for perceived task interdependence in model 2. 62 Table 6. Regression Analysis: Predicting Perceived Awareness by Perceived Task Interdependence, Communication Frequency, and TeamSCOPE Usage ezfiggldeent Perceived Awareness Independent Model Variables 1 2 3 4 5 5 Task lnterdependenc .. . .. . e (Tl) .422 .313 .505 .510 .431 .329 Communication .. . Frequency (CF) .179 .461 .260 .518 TeamSCOPE . Usage (TU) -.235i -.239* -.305 -.257’r Tl x CF -.396' -.420' TI x TU -023 .037 CF x TU .044 R2 .253 .335 .277 .273 .333 .412 F 7955'“ 7.765’“ 8.057'" 5.257” 6.824“ 4.431“ AR2 .083 (F=53.77“') .001 (F=.059) .079 (F = 1.739) N=50 ' * significant at .05 level, ** significant at .01 level, *** significant at .001 level I Moderately significant at .10 level 63 Table 7. Regression Analysis: Predicting Perceived Awareness by Perceived Task Interdependence, Communication Frequency, and TeamSCOPE Usage (Group Level) Dependent Variable Independent Model Variables 1 2 3 4 5 Task lnterdependenc e (Tl) .570 -273 .313T .970 .545 Communication Frequency (CF) .257 1.759 .499 TeamSCOPE Usage (TU) -.300 -.372 -.467 Perceived Awareness Tl x CF 4220 TI x TU -.139 CF x TU R2 .573 .713 .321 . 635 .743 F 2.734 2.433 3.279 1.733 2.393 AR2 .135 .014 N = 7 * significant at .05 level, ** significant at .01 level, *** significant at .001 level 1 Moderately significant at .10 level 64 In summary, perceived task interdependence was a significant predictor for perceived awareness level. H3 was supported. In accordance with H4a, the effect of communication frequency on awareness level was positive and significant. However, TeamSCOPE usage was negatively associated with awareness level. Therefore, H4b was not supponed. A moderator effect of task structure was found between communication frequency and perceived awareness. As H6a proposed, the negative sign of the interaction term (OD x CF) indicated that the effect of communication frequency on awareness level decreased when perceived task interdependence increased. The interaction effect between perceived task interdependence and TeamSCOPE usage was not significant. H6b was not supported. Finally, among the 6 models tested above, the all-inclusive model, which involved task interdependence, communication frequency, TeamSCOPE usage, and the three two-way interaction terms (Table 6, Model 6), explained the most variance of perceived awareness level (R2 = 412). However, in this model, only three items made statistically significant contribution in predicting awareness: task interdependence, communication frequency, and the interaction between perceived task interdependence and communication frequency. The model that contained these three terms (Table 6, Model 2) explained the second most variance (R2 = .336). An F test was performed to test if the difference of R2 between Model 2 and Model 6 was statistically significant. The result showed that the increase in R2 was not significant (F=1.505, p= .230). The addition of 65 TeamSCOPE usage and additional interaction terms did not add to the predicting power of Model 2. The Effects of Task interdependence on Awareness Channel Reliance H3a: Higher level of perceived task interdependence will lead to greater perceived reliance on synchronous communication channel for awareness purpose. H3b: Perceived task interdependence will have no effect on the extent to which asynchronous communication media is relied upon for awareness purpose. In support of H3a, perceived task interdependence was found to positively contribute to the perceived degree to which distant meetings were relied upon as awareness channels (r = .566, p < .001). Results of group level analyses also supported this hypothesis with moderate statistical significance (r = .734, p = .060) H3b was tested against the perceived reliance on TeamSCOPE and emails as awareness channels. In support of H3b, no significant effect of perceived task interdependence was found on perceived reliance on TeamSCOPE for awareness purposes. However, results showed that perceived task interdependence had a negative effect (r = -.343, p = .015) on the degree to which participants perceived to rely on emails to stay updated with distant teammates’ activities. H3b was partially supported. 66 The Effects of Awareness Channel Reliance on Awareness H5: Greater reliance on synchronous communication channel for awareness purpose will lead to higher level of perceived awareness. Results showed that the perceived degree to which synchronous meetings with distant members were relied upon as an awareness channel was positively _ and significantly contributed to perceived level of awareness (r = .661, p < .001). H5 was supported. Analysis at the group level also supported H5. Additionally, results showed no statically significant relationship between the perceived level of awareness and the perceived reliance on asynchronous awareness channels, including email and TeamSCOPE. The Effect of task interdependence and awareness on Trust H7: Higher level of perceived task interdependence will lead to higher level of trust. Perceived task interdependence was found to contribute positively and significantly to perceived trust on distant teammates (r = .454, p = .001). H7 was supported. Analysis at the group level also found this effect to be significant (r = .828, p = .021). H8: Higher level of perceived awareness will lead to higher level of trust. Results found that the more participants perceived to be aware of distant partners’ progress, the higher degree of trust they had in them (r = .665, p < 67 .001). H8 was supported. The strong relationship between awareness and trust was also found at the group level. Additionally, multiple regression analyses were conducted to test if the effects of awareness and perceived task interdependence on trust were additive or interactive (see Table 8). Model 1 in Table 8 assessed the effect of perceived awareness as the sole predictor of trust. 44.2% of variance of trust could be explained by perceived awareness (R2 = .442). Model 2 assessed the additive effects of perceived task interdependence and awareness on trust. The two-term main effect model was statistically significant (F = 19.894, p <.001). However, while the effect of perceived awareness was still significant (8 = .579, p < .001), the effect of perceived task interdependence was not (B = .186, p = .136). The two-term main effect model explained 46.9% of the trust variance (R2 = .469). Comparing Model 2 to Model 1, there was a 2.7% increase in R2. An F test was conducted to see if the difference was significant (von Eye 8 Schuster, 1998). The F statistic indicated that the difference was not significant (F = 2.136, p = .151 ). In addition, the interaction model (Table 8, Model 3) did not explain more variance of trust than the main effect model, either (AR2 = 0). Multiple regression analyses at the group level also found similar results (Table 9). Awareness was a strong and significant predictor of trust ([3 = .764, p = .008), while the effect of perceived task interdependence was much weaker and not significant ([3 = .268, p = .159). 68 Table 8. Regression Analysis: Predicting Trust by Perceived Awareness and Perceived Task Interdependence Dependent Variable Trust Model 1 2 3 Independent Standardized Standardized Standardized Variables Coefficient Coefficient Coefficient Awareness (AW) 665*“ .579*** .588 Perceived task interdependence (Tl) .186 .199 AW x Tl -.019 R2 .442 .459 .459 F 36.458*** 1 9.894*** 12 .968*** N = 50 * significant at .05 level, ** significant at .01 level, *** significant at .001 level 69 Table 9. Regression Analysis: Predicting Trust by Perceived Awareness and Perceived Task Interdependence (Group Level) Dependent Variable Trust Model 1 2 3 Independent Standardized Standardized Standardized Variables Coefficient Coefficient Coefficient Awareness (AW) .960*** .764" .693* Perceived task interdependence (TI) .268 .269 AW x Tl -.113 R2 .922 .955 .953 F 59.461 *** 43.066“ 26.276* N = 7 * significant at .05 level, ** significant at .01 level, *** significant at .001 level 70 The Effect of Task Interdependence and Awareness on Solution Agreement H9: Higher level of perceived task interdependence will lead to higher level of perceived solution agreement. H10: Higher level of perceived awareness will lead to higher level of perceived solution agreement. Results showed no significant relationship between perceived solution agreement and perceived task interdependence (r = .097, p = .503). Also, the effect of perceived awareness on perceived solution agreement was not significant ([3 = .055, p = .706). Both H9 and H10 were rejected. Results of analyses at the group level concurred those of individual level analyses. Research model with results was presented in Figure 6. In addition, summary of results was provided in Table 10. Insert Figure 6 about here. 71 Table 10. Summary of Hypotheses and Results effect on the extent to which asynchronous communication media is relied upon for awareness purpose. Hypotheses Results H1: Higher level of perceived task interdependence Supported will lead to higher level of perceived awareness. H2a: Higher level of perceived task interdependence Supported will lead to higher communication frequency. H2b: Higher level of perceived task Not supported interdependence will lead to more TeamSCOPE usage. H3a: Higher level of perceived task interdependence Supported will lead to greater perceived reliance on synchronous communication channel for awareness purpose. H3b: Perceived task interdependence will have no Supported for TeamSCOPE, but rejected for emails. H4a: Higher communication frequency will lead to higher level of perceived awareness. Supponed H4b: More TeamSCOPE usage will lead to higher level of perceived awareness. Not supported H5: Greater reliance on synchronous communication Channel for awareness purpose will lead to higher level of perceived awareness. Supponed H6a: The effect of communication frequency on level of awareness will be weaker for people who perceive higher degree of task interdependence. Supponed H6b: The effect of TeamSCOPE usage on level of awareness will be weaker for people who perceive higher degree of task interdependence. Not supported H7: Higher level of perceived task interdependence will lead to higher level of trust. Supponed H8: Higher level of perceived awareness will lead to higher level of trust. Supponed H9: Higher level of perceived task interdependence will lead to higher level of perceived solution agreement. Not supported H10: Higher level of perceived awareness will lead to higher level of perceived solution agreement. Not supported 72 CHAPTER 5 DISCUSSION AND CONCLUSION The results of this study have shed light on the role of awareness in group processes. Specifically, they provided insights into the effects of task structure and media use on the level of awareness, and the effects of awareness on trust and solution agreement. The limitations of this study as well as the implications of the results are discussed below. The Effects of Task Interdependence on Media Behaviors Task interdependence was hypothesized to have effects on communication frequency, TeamSCOPE usage, as well as the degrees of reliance on different channels for awareness purposes. As discussed previously, communication barriers in virtual teams make interacting among dispersed team members difficult. The results indicated that perceived task interdependence was a significant factor that motivated virtual team participants to overcome the communication barriers. The result of the analysis at the group level also found a positive relationship between perceived task interdependence and communication frequency. However, no statistically significant relationship was found between perceived task interdependence and TeamSCOPE usage. Using TeamSCOPE was relatively low-effort activities. It did not require prior coordination, as in the 73 case of synchronous communication, or extra cognitive efforts, such as crafting an email message. As a result, there was no barrier that would discriminate participants with different task requirements to use TeamSCOPE. The relationship between perceived task interdependence and perceived reliance on different awareness channels provided useful insights. In this study, participants were provided a set of communication tools to choose from to carry out their intra-team interaction. Results found that participants who perceived higher task interdependence also preferred synchronous meeting with distant members for their awareness needs. The result was in accordance with media richness theory. In addition, participants who perceived higher task interdependence was less likely to rely on emails as their awareness channel. Together, the results demonstrated that, given the Choices and under high task demands, people would rely on synchronous tools for updates. As Olson and Teasley (1996) pointed out that CSCW research was predominantly populated with one-medium team studies. In those studies, teams were provided only one type of media and performance of teams using different media was compared. However, in corporate life, people are often supported with a variety of communication tools. By simulating a medium-abundance work environment, this study was able to provide insights on media Choice in virtual teams. This finding is particularty important in a virtual team context because of the difficulty to arrange real-time meetings for distributed group. In addition to task requirement, cultural factors might also contribute to these results. 74 Particularly, language barrier was an issue. Even though all non-US participants possessed English language skills, the degrees of proficiency were not equal among members in each subgroup. In three of the seven teams, the one that had the best English skills was not the most knowledgeable member in his or her subgroup. As a result, email correspondences between locations were slowed because they had to be routed through the member with better English skills. This mismatch of language proficiency and professional expertise created an unnecessary communication bottleneck and rendered asynchronous Channel inefficient. On the other hand, during real-time meetings, since all members were presented, the routing time due to language barrier was reduced to minimal. It was then more efficient to clarify progress and problems between the two subgroups. This implication should be taken with cautions. One important factor that influenced the feasibility and efficiency of real-time meeting was the two-location team structure. As mentioned before, all teams participated in this study were composed of students from two sites. It was relatively easy to coordinate meeting schedule between two subgroups as opposed to three or more sites. In addition, there were multiple students in each location. If a participant had to miss a meeting, his or her local teammates could pass along the information. With this local safety net, it was not necessary for meeting schedules to accommodate the individual schedules of all team members. Virtual teams with members distributed in more locations and with less local teammates may find it 75 more difficult to arrange real-time meetings and, therefore, rely more on asynchronous communication channels. The Effects of Task Interdependence and Media behavior on Awareness A major focus of this study was to examine the antecedents of awareness. Task structure, communication frequency, groupware use, and awareness channel utilization were proposed to affect the perceived level of awareness. Results showed that perceived task interdependence, communication frequency, and reliance on cross-location meetings had positive effects on awareness. TeamSCOPE usage and reliance on emails for awareness were found to be negatively associated with level of awareness. Perceived task interdependence was positively associated with awareness. The more distant partners’ input was needed for the project, the greater the impact would be if distant partners did not deliver on schedule. Therefore, virtual team participants were more driven to stay updated on their distant partners’ progress when they perceived higher degree of task interdependence. The analysis at the group level concurred with the result. Again, perceived task interdependence created awareness demands that prompted virtual team participants to engage in awareness conducts. Communication frequency was found to positively contribute to awareness as proposed with moderate significance. However, the strength of its effect was affected by perceived task interdependence. The influences of communication frequency on awareness were lower for participants who 76 perceived higher task interdependence. It suggested that more communication incidents were needed in order to meet increased awareness demands due to higher perceived task interdependence. On the other hand, the negative relationship between TeamSCOPE usage and perceived level of awareness was not expected. In correlation analysis as well as the multiple regression models with perceived task interdependence, this negative relationship were at lease moderately significant. The same trend was also found in the regression analyses conducted at the group level. These results indicated that participants who used TeamSCOPE more were less aware of distant teammates’ activities. Following the research model, which implied that awareness is a function of groupware usage, the result may be explained by tool proficiency. TeamSCOPE usage included all page requests made by users. Participants who knew where to find the information in TeamSCOPE would make fewer requests than those who did not. Therefore, high TeamSCOPE usage might indicate less effective search and lower awareness. However, the effect of tool proficiency should be mitigated in the long run. Frustrated users would drop out using TeamSCOPE or learn to use it efficiently. Both should lead to lower page requests over time. An alternative way to explain the results would be to reverse the implied causal relationship in the research model. Instead of having perceived awareness as a function of groupware usage, we could posit groupware usage as a function of perceived awareness. When members were not able to stay updated on distant teammates’ progress, they were more likely to initiate 77 awareness conducts via available media and tools. This alternative explanation made sense especially for TeamSCOPE usage, since TeamSCOPE, as an awareness channel, required the least amount of effort from users to acquire information. When awareness needs arose, logging on TeamSCOPE would be the easiest action to take. It was also unobtrusive to distant partners and, therefore, a reasonable first step to address awareness needs. However, due to its limitation in interactivity, TeamSCOPE may not be the most effective awareness channel and, therefore, users’ awareness needs were not necessary satisfied. In other words, the usage of TeamSCOPE was more likely an indicator for awareness demand rather than awareness supply. The ambiguity of the relationship between TeamSCOPE usage and perceived awareness invites further discussion on awareness demand and satisfaction. The alternative explanation posits that groupware usage was influenced by the extent to which awareness demand was satisfied. When awareness supply does not meet the demand, more awareness conducts will be initiated. In the research model, awareness demand was implied in task interdependence. It was reasoned that due to increasing coordination needs for highly interdependent task, virtual team members need more frequent updates on each other’s progress. The demand for awareness was considered to be embedded in the task structure. However, other group structural factors may also influence awareness demand. For future research, it should help to clarify the relationship between group structural variables and media behavior by clearly identifying awareness demand in the model. The addition of awareness demand 78 in the model also fit well with the moderating effect of perceived task interdependence on the relationship between communication frequency and perceived awareness. The higher the demand, the more communication events were needed. A revised research model that includes awareness demand is presented in Figure 7. Insert Figure 7 here. Additionally, results also showed that the degree to which participants relied on different media for awareness had influences on perceived level of awareness. In accordance with hypothesis, the more participants relied on synchronous meeting with distant partners to stay updated, the more they were aware of the current state of distant members’ progress. On the other hand, the effects of asynchronous channels on awareness were not conclusive. Results showed there was no significant relationship between perceived level of awareness and perceived degree of reliance on email or TeamSCOPE for awareness purpose. The differences may contribute to the different medium characteristics. Most importantly, instant feedback capability afforded by synchronous meeting greatly facilitated team members to satisfy their awareness needs. The Effects of Task Interdependence and Awareness on Trust Both perceived task interdependence and awareness were proposed to have positive effects on trust. Results of this study supported the hypotheses. Higher level of perceived task interdependence and perceived awareness led to 79 higher level of trust on distant teammates. However, an F test on R2 of different regression models showed that perceived task interdependence did not add a statistically significant contribution to trust. It was safe to conclude that there was no directly effect of perceived task interdependence on trust. Rather, perceived task interdependence may influence trust through motivating virtual team members to engage in awareness conducts. This result implicated that awareness is a valuable intermediate construct that may provide a link between group structural variables and group process outcomes. The relationship between trust and awareness is worthy of further discussion. As noted previously, awareness may facilitate trust development by dissuading social loafing, promoting reliable and dependable conducts, and allowing confirmation on expectations. The implicit assumptions for the above effects to take place are that awareness is mutual and transparent. When awareness is mutual, all members have equal resources and authority to monitor others. Each team member can know as much about the other members as the other team members can know about him or her. When awareness is transparent, all team members know what information their partners can have access to. Both assumptions are important for trust building. When awareness is unilateral, it exposes the less privileged parties to the risk of being exploited by the opportunistic behaviors of the privileged members. When awareness is not transparent, team members do not have a good idea if any or which part of their work output will be visible. It may encourage social loafing or decrease social facilitation effect. In this study, a mutual and transparent awareness environment 80 was ensured by (1) the lateral structure of the design teams, which established equal privileges and, (2) the trainings on media and TeamSCOPE, which familiarized participants with awareness or track-keeping related features in various tools. The Effects of Task Interdependence and Awareness on Solution Agreement Task interdependence and awareness were proposed to positively contribute to solution agreement via increasing task-related communication and promoting a group conforming behavior. There were no significant effect of perceived task interdependence or awareness on solution agreement was found at the individual level or the group level. It suggested that the information and normative influences that were facilitated by task structure and awareness were not as strong as proposed in the process of reaching consensus. However, there were a couple of factors that might prevent the hypotheses to be tested fairly. First, the scores of solution agreement might be skewed. This scale asked if the participants thought the team solution addressed the design problem and if they agree with it. Participants, whose grade depended on the final project outcome, may incline to give high score on both items based on the suspicion that the grading faculty members may review their questionnaires. Statistics showed that the mean score of solution agreement was the highest among all latent variables and the variation of the scale was the lowest (Mean = 4.42, SD = .59). 81 Second, the temporal dimension of project work was not considered. Although solution agreement, task structure, and awareness level were all measured at the end of the project, teams decided on their final solution at an earlier point in the project life. The virtual design teams in this project followed a general design methodology. They began by defining the problem, followed by proposing solutions, choosing a solution, and then finally implementing the solution. Usually, the decision on final solution was made around the mid-point of the project or even earlier to leave enough time for implementation. Consequently, solution agreement could not be predicted by task structure and awareness level measured at the end of the project. Instead, the measured dependencies and awareness between subgroups may be in part resulted form the selected solution or the way it was implemented. To better assess the effects of task structure and awareness, they should be measured at the time when teams made their decisions on design solutions. Limitations, Implications and Suggestions for Future Research Limitations This research had several limitations. First, although it was intended to simulate real industrial project environments, these were student teams. Their risks and rewards were grade-based. Also, they did not have the same organizational support as professionals. For example, some of them were not able to have access to the communication facilities whenever they needed. The generizability of the study is limited. 82 Another issue is the small sample size. There were only 53 subjects in this study. Due to the limited sample size, more sophisticated analytical tools, such as structural equation modeling, could not be applied. In addition, there were only seven design teams participated in the study. The small number of teams made it impractical to obtain statistic power of analysis at the group level. In addition, the two-subgroup formation with multiple members in each subgroup was a specific team structure. As mentioned above, real-time meetings that included the whole team were more feasible when scheduling involved only two sites. Also, the multiple-member subgroup arrangement allowed one to rely on his or her local teammates for project updates if one missed a communication event or did not log on to TeamSCOPE. Although it provided a safety net for virtual team participants to stay in the loop, it also made it difficult to determine the relationship between media use and awareness. The group dynamic of this specific team formation should be different from those without local subgroups and those with more subgroups. Moreover, not all teams were created equal despite our efforts. Several disparities could be found in these teams. The communication infrastructures each team had to work with were different. It was more difficult to access video conferencing facilities for some teams than others. It sometimes forced them to change meeting schedules or opt for other media. The windows of time available to work synchronously across locations were also different from team to team. Some teams were not even able to work out a schedule to have all members present at cross-location meetings due to other obligations, such as Classes. 83 Finally, participants were predominantly male. Only 12 of the 53 participants were female. Gender balance was difficult to achieve across teams and across sites. The above disparities all had implications on media use and awareness conducts. Furthermore, there were several issues regarding measurements. Communication frequency was collected from participants’ self-reports. There was no mechanism to check their reliability. Also, the types of media used in each communication event were not factored in when calculated the communication frequency due to the imparity of media accessibility. As discussed above, not all tools were available at all time to all teams. As a result, the Choices of media might be affected by the communication infrastructure rather than perceived task interdependence as presented in the research model. However, using only the counts of communication incidents prevented analysis on effects of different media. Additionally, the internal consistency of scales, tested using coefficient alpha, was not ideal. It might in part due to the differences in English proficiency of participants. Also, as discussed above, the measurement of solution agreement might be skewed. Future studies of long-term work teams may take into account the temporal dimension of project work in administering data collection. Overall, the externally validity of the result might suffer from using student teams and the disparities of the teams. Also, since it was the first time that all students participated in a virtual team, the results of this study might not be 84 applied to virtual teams with experienced participants (Hollingshead, McGrath, 8 O’Connor, 1993). Finally, it should also be cautious when comparing the results of this study with others that had different types of team formation. Implications for Theories and Future Research The purpose of this study is to integrate the research on awareness in the CSCW and HCI communities with the literature of work group study, social psychology and computer-mediated communication to further understand group processes in virtual teams. The results suggested several theoretical implications. First, awareness can be a valuable intermediary construct in understanding group processes. As discussed above, awareness provided the link between perceived task interdependence and trust. Future research may continue this inquiry to further test the effects of awareness on other group outcomes, such as job satisfaction and group performance. In addition, this study expanded the research of awareness to include social psychological aspects. Particularly, perceived awareness was linked to reducing social Ioafing and increasing social facilitation. These effects ultimately led to trust. However, as discussed above, these social psychological implications were based on the assumptions that awareness was both mutual and transparent. It is then important to investigate how to meet these assumptions in terms of group structure, task design, organizational control mechanism, and media support. 85 Moreover, this study focused only on activity awareness. The effects of other types of awareness were not investigated. For example, perspective awareness may be related to establishing group norms and resolving conflicts. There is also a need to development more instruments to measure different types of awareness. Finally, future studies on awareness should take into account the temporal dimension of project development. As discussed above, design teams may go through different stages during their project life. The interaction among group members in an earlier stage may influence how they conduct their work later on. Especially for zero-history virtual teams, no pre-existing consensual norms or rules to govern communication and information exchange. Awareness deficit experienced at the beginning of the project may lead a team to avoid mutual dependence. It suggests awareness level may have implications on how teams Choose to divide their work and coordinate their actions. In other words, coordination mechanism may be a function of awareness level at the early stage of the project. Implications for Groupware Design To offer better awareness service by collaborative tools, this study suggested that active mechanisms for collecting information on project status should be considered. As discussed above, the negative relationship between TeamSCOPE usage and awareness level might indicate TeamSCOPE, as a low- effort, unobtrusive channel, was the first place to go when awareness needs 86 arose. However, the utility of TeamSCOPE as an awareness channel depended on how actively it was used by the whole group (Jang et al., 2002; Markus, 1990). When there was no new information, participants could not tell if it was due to lack of progress orjust delayed upload. The differences between the information available in TeamSCOPE and the current status of project rendered TeamSCOPE a less effective awareness tool. To improve on the supply of awareness information, groupware systems need to incorporate tools that can actively collect project-related information. For example, a survey via emails regarding the progress of various subtasks can be administered by TeamSCOPE periodically between cross-location meetings. It should be noted that it was by design that TeamSCOPE did not actively collect awareness information (Charles Steinfield et al., 1999). The purpose was to minimize users’ effort and be unobtrusive. Groupware designers should take into account the tradeoff between rich information and extra user effort (Konrad Tollmar et al., 1996). It is suggested to offer some flexibility in the information collecting functions to allow users to reach the balance between the effort and information themselves. Implications for Practice Some practical implications for virtual teams managers and participants can be drawn from the results. For organizations and practitioners, it is worth paying attention to the issue of awareness in the context of virtual teams. Staying updated on remote teammates’ progress significantly contributed to trust development in virtual teams. To enhance awareness, task structure and media 87 use should be considered. The results showed that perceived task interdependence positively contributed to awareness. Accordingly, to structurally facilitate awareness, the design of virtual team and their tasks should embed mutual dependence among team members. However, high dependence also creates high demand for awareness and more frequent communication is needed. Virtual team should be provided with adequate support of communication tools. Specifically, relying on synchronous channels was found have positive effects on awareness. Organizational support, such as easy access to facility and flexible working hours, may greatly reduce the barriers to use synchronous communication tools and, therefore, facilitate awareness conducts. Finally, as discussed above, the positive social psychological effects of awareness and awareness conducts are based on the assumption that awareness is mutual and transparent. Tracking and monitoring mechanisms in virtual teams should be set up carefully. A violation of the assumption may lead to social loafing, invasion of privacy, or other undesired results. Conclusion The focus of this study was to investigate the antecedents and effects of awareness in geographically dispersed project teams. The results of this research indicated that task structure significantly influenced the level of awareness. When the knowledge, skills, and work output from distant partners were required to accomplish the project, virtual team members were more likely 88 to stay aware of remote partners’ activities. Perceived task interdependence motivated virtual team participants to overcome various communication barriers to maintain awareness. However, high perceived task interdependence also indicated high awareness demands and more effort was required to satisfy those demands. It was demonstrated by the moderator effect of perceived task interdependence on the relationship between communication frequency and level of awareness. The effect size of communication frequency on awareness decreased when perceived task interdependence increased. The relationship between media usage and awareness were more dynamic than the hypothesis indicated. Although communication frequency was positively contributed to awareness level, usage of TeamSCOPE was negatively associated with awareness. The casual direction suggested in the research model, awareness as a function of media usage, could only explain part of the findings. The results suggested that lack of awareness would drive virtual team members to use available media to stay updated. Therefore, low-effort awareness channels will more likely be associated with unsatisfied awareness demands. On the other hand, although direct contact with distant teammates often provided more awareness information, it required more coordination or cognitive efforts. Accordingly, in order to facilitate awareness, it is suggested to increase the awareness information supply for low-effort channels and to reduce the barriers of using high-effort channels. Several implications on groupware design and virtual team management were discussed above. 89 In conclusion, despite its limitation, this study provided several insights on the relationships between awareness, task structure, media use, and trust in virtual teams. One purpose of this research is to fuse the concept of awareness with the extensive body of literature of group study. Results of this project showed that awareness provided a link between task structure and trust. lt demonstrated that awareness could be an important variable of group processes. To further clarify the role of awareness in the context of distributed groups, more investigations on its relationship with other group structural variables and group outcome variable are needed. 90 fit, Figures 91 Figure 2. TeamSCOPE: Activity Summary Screen .,‘ . _ _ __ __ .1._.,.....7 wqmu..w. v'.,".n‘.‘,‘( - . .. ‘ @Jlranlfitfl’lzlcam I t "'IC ""LufiAAf-‘elf A; ~32 - - ~_,.,,.‘I _; f . j . .1011] f ) V D 765.7% Sfém’ [Aclmty :1 Got [$19233] Administrative Team Recent Activity Vieutng matches 1 - 10: There are no new actin'b'es since your last fish. Egg . Actlvltv _ll_e Date I BenPfafl‘VVVVVVV read ‘ 'V ' RezL'pioadineproblem ' ' """““axoe’eemezoooj endgeagjzageplaaaed” 'VVVVVV"VV'dataloorausoor-nxt ' xeearoaxzeooi En‘k Goodman A‘dounloaded studentszooomulum'VV V V VV V V V MxV'VosVVoonsdzooo; Erik comm V read V V V ' TemnSC‘OPE 0.9.22 releasedVVMV“ V' mezeemw Charles Steinfield read V TeamSCOPE 0.9.22 released ' ' V V V ” " VV V '0et27VV1V751'V8V 2000. Tim Hinds VV V V dotfitloaded ' ' '.sntdemszooorau.lmn ' VV V V ' V V 70cié§1235£2000§ ChyugYangVJaugV tead 1, V V " Is the email notification red-m1” 'V ' VV V Vo'th'iLiVViOfisaVzooo ChynngngJangV {delete-eaten" “ data oorau ” ” ‘ V :oVe'tV"1"7"io:32V V2000 -’ Ben pee V V V posted TeamSCOPE 0.9.2: released V V V V V V V‘ ‘ “"de‘t'13‘i‘2'd‘9éooo'; Chsngatigiang uploaded V V ismdautleOOfallJttm V I V Oct 1VOVVlV22'582000: " Expressed in your currently selected time zone: L'SfMtchlgan L? F!» 92 Figure 3. TeamSCOPE: File Activity History .7 W" (9“ I» .fii‘fi'il.‘.:"‘. I." -' W -- L'fl'aSu-s‘i-‘J %'-'-h'.-vm 'w-‘r-r ww-mI-eauwunran. L‘ a ”v:- e.-. . damn-xx; ‘3; ..-. Nah‘lo l" '—-.".P.‘.':‘.'.'.'."I'.'-'p l'o‘v -'- v I I "- '_'_V_‘ A'-‘.V.'.‘.’s'.~ r‘I I .~- '2’ """ ' E '."’.'.'. J] V ‘ feafifffim ldss :_l _G_°'_i w Administrative Team Folders s____hared 2580M Viewing attributes of file: shared folder Class Assignments Mucus. tgs "V Attribute " 'u..-—.. M ._--- I Value User Chyng't'ang . Jang bu lie..- ._ - . __ lLast access 3 2 days ago VV Last ehange: :414 9 days ago User ' Vprmleges: read/write Team 'prmleges .OVut51de1‘ V g - Vor1vxleces _ .. ”read-only 13 “We. read-only ; Enk Goodman :Erik Goodman ; éChyngYang Jang ChyngYans dang new Done 1.----- 1 932:. 1T1m Hinds l 1 1. wad—“a. wm~— —.— —--—--- .._.._---... _._... @9@Q®@®®.® co_mrr____1mt scanty VII—CW do_m_tl__oadr rglace Activity idownloaded idownloaded iido'stv'nloaded m_ove r_____cnamc co d__e1ete ac__c__css Date 1 : 1 goa 15 Class Assimments/focus igs 1345 . 3.1999 : be 15 ; :Class A331gnrnents/focus 135313 35 . §1999 igOct 14 _'Class Assigmtents/focus 135 16:10 . 1999 Flle -...-‘.--——_-——- _-_ —. read comment read comment 93 1 0ct-V14m 3Class Assmnments/focus 1gs:12 21 g1999 - 4.. _.-—--..... 4 v.— -.~ F-.- .. ~..... _.._..- -.. _.- -. -. .... - ( be 111------ Class Assignments/focus s1g§ 12 21 1 §l999 .L. 9v" ”’41- .139 __ Figure 4. TeamSCOPE: Message Board gr. lcam‘KOPl: team when . - 1' .. .- . . , ‘3; __..‘ " ‘_ -|D|_x_j. Zg‘gfinyK/‘i [Messaoes .1] 60' [Help M Administrative Team [ Index ] Re: TeamSCOPE 0.9.17 released [works now!) by Ben Pfaff at Thu Jun 29 11:42:24 2000 EDT Yow! lt worked this time. [am happy. for a moment. Time for lunch. Viewed by: Erik Goodman, ChyngYang Jang, Ingrid Mulder, Ben Pfaff. Charles Steinfield [ Replv ] Thread Index: 0 TeamSCOPE 0.9.17 released by Ben Pfaff at Tue Jun 27 14:50:03 2000 EDT 0 TeamSCOPE 0.9.1? released by Charles Ste'mfield at Wed Jun 28 07:44:19 2000 EDT 1- Re: TeamSCOPE 0.9.17 releasedjresent for testing] by Ben Pfaft‘ at Thu Jun 29 11:35:42 2000 EDT - Re; TeamSCOPE 0.9.17 released [works now!] by Ben Pfaff at Thu Jun 29 11:42:24 2000 EDT 0 Is the emfl notification working; by Charles Steinfield at Thu Jun 29 08:55:59 2000 EDT I Is the email notification “orients; by Ben Pfaff at Thu Jun 29 11:44:16 2000 EDT 7 , fi Q! Mac Santfitrteac ‘21 a 94 Figure 5. TeamSCOPE: Login Status, Login Record and Usage Pattern 1s; I.‘.Im‘~( um: [ramSwift-Ho1Ur(uu.llh)ldl~vFyvjt\1lmrnunrne'nh lithium p 1 n :9 1 ;] film 2‘11 Administrative Team __ . . ,Vw‘nf95mm’y .. L‘sernnme Last login iUsage pattern . ..... . . .. “25...... _. I 1 S andn'ess Jan 28 2003 09:44 i ifl SF """""" ‘ 1 “5126 3 5 hip Feb 09 2002 21:52 ; 1m 5' ..5‘ . V S j goodman Apr 28 2002 14:21 1 1 g f sF i -........_... .31”. : S . binds 111102 20031051 § 1% g : 5F 5 ” 6126 g g S : jangchyn Dec 10 2003 22:39; VIBE 5 . 5r 3 1 3 : lloyd Oct 07 2002 1521 1m 1 : Sr . 5125 _. . 5:33] 1' SF steinfie Nov 19 2003 22:43 95 Figure 6. Research Model with Results Perceived Task Interdependence .454”-1 -.396" 2 .6655” Trust 2133’ CF .299' TU -------------- N Perceived Awareness .566“ RD .661'" _ .473'" _ i Solution "T" Agreement ———————————————————————————— —’ CF: Communication frequency TU: TeamSCOPE usage RD: Perceived reliance on distant meetings for awareness purposes Dashed line indicates proposed relationship not supported by the results. * significant at .05 level ** significant at .01 level *** significant at .001 level Note: 1. The number above each arrow was the pair-wise correlation coefficient, excepting those being noted othenNise. 2. Standardized coefficient for the interaction term (Tl x CF) in regression model #2 in Table 6. 96 Figure 7. Revised Research Model Group structure variables Group outcome variables Perceived awareness demand Media Behavior Perceived Awareness 97 REFERENCES Adler, N. J. (1991). International Dimensions of Organizational Behavior. Boston, MA: PWS-Kent. Adler, P. S. (1995). Interdepartmental interdependence and coordination: The case of the design/manufacturing interface. Organization Science, 6(2), 147-167. Alge, B. J., Wiethoff, C., & Klein, H. J. (2003). When does the medium matter? Knowledge-building experiences and opportunities in decision-making teams. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 91(1), 26-37. Allen, T. J. (1977). Managing the Flow of Technology: Technology Transfer and Dissemination of Technological lnfonnation Within the R&D Organization. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Applegate, L. M. (1999). In search of a new organizational model. In G. DeSanctis & J. Fulk (Eds.), Shaping Organization Form: Communication, Connection, and Community (pp. 33-70). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications. Argote, L., & McGrath, J. E. (1993). Group processes in organizations: Continuity and change. In C. L. Cooper & l. T. Robertson (Eds.), lntemational Review of Industrial and Organizational Psychology (pp. 333-389). Chichester, UK: Wiley. Aulakh, P. S., Kotabe, M., & Sahay, A. (1996). Trust and performance in cross- border marketing partnerships: A behavioral approach. Journal of International Business Studies, 27(5), 1005-1032. Bales, R. F. (1958). Task roles and social roles in problem-solving groups. In E. E. Maccoby, T. M. Newcomb & E. L. Hartley (Eds.), Social Psychology (3rd ed., pp. 437-447). New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston. Barua, A., Lee, C.-H. S., & Whinston, A. B. (1999). Incentives and computing systems for team-based organizations: A complementarity perspective. In G. DeSanctis & J. Fulk (Eds.), Shaping Organization Form: Communication, Connection, and Community (pp. 173-209). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications. Bell, B. S., & Kozlowski, S. W. J. (2002). A typology of virtual teams: Implications for effective leadership. Group & Organization Management, 27(1), 14-49. 98 Bentley, R., Appelt, W., Busbash, U., Hinrichs, E., Kerr, 0., Sikkel, K., et al. (1997). Basic support for cooperative work on the World Wide Web. International Journal of Human-Computer Studies(46), 827-846. Bentley, R., Horstmann, T., 8 Trevor, J. (1997). The World Wide Web as enabling technology for CSCW: The case of BSCW. In R. Bentley, U. Busbach, D. Kerr 8 K. Sikkel (Eds.), Groupware and the Wortd Wide Web (pp. 1-24). Dordrecht, the Netherlands: Kluwer Academic Publishers. Bentley, R., Hughes, J. A., Randall, D., Rodden, T., Sawyer, P., Shapiro, D., et al. (1992). Ethnographically-inforrned systems design for air traffic control. Conference on Computer-Supported Cooperative Work. Oct. 31 - Nov. 4. Toronto, Canada. Berger, P. L., & Luckmann, T. (1967). The Social Construction of Reality. New York: Anchor Books. Bettenhausen, K. L. (1991). Five years of groups research: What we have learned and what needs to be addressed. Journal of Management, 17, 345-381. Blau, P. M. (1964). Exchange and Power in Social Life. New York: Wiley. Bly, S., Harrison, 8. R., 8 Irwin, S. (1993). Media spaces: Bringing people together in a video, audio, and computing environment. Communications of the ACM, 36(1), 28-47. Boland, R. J., Schwartz, D. G., & Tenkasi, R. V. (1992). Sharing perspectives in distributed decision making. CSCW '92. November. Toronto, Canada. Boutellier, R., Gassmann, O., Macho, H., & Roux;, M. (1998). Management of dispersed product development teams: The role of information technologies. R & D Management, 28(1), 13-25. Brass, D. J. (1985). Technology and the structuring of jobs: Employee satisfaction, performance, and influence. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 35, 216-240. Bromiley, P., & Cummings, L. L. (1995). Transaction costs in organizations with trust. In R. Bies, B. Sheppard & R. Lewicki (Eds.), Research on Negotiation in Organizations. Greenwich, CT: JAl Press. Brown, C. L. (1999). "Do the right thing:” Diverging effects of accountability in a managerial context. Marketing Science, 18(3), 230-246. Brown, R. (2000). Group Processes: Dynamics Within And Between Groups (Second ed.). Oxford: Blackwell Publishers Ltd. 99 Butler, J. K. (1991). Towards understanding and measuring conditions of trust: Evolution of a conditions of trust inventory. Journal of Management, 17(17), 643-663. Campion, M. A., Medsker, G. A., 8 Higgs, C. A. (1993). Relations between work group characteristics and effectiveness: Implications for designing effective work groups. Personnel Psychology, 46, 823-850. Campion, M. A., Papper, E. M., 8 Medsker, G. J. (1996). Relations between work team characteristics and effectiveness: A replication and extension. Personnel Psychology, 49, 429-452. Castore, C. H., 8 Murnighan, K. J. (1978). Determinants of support for group decisions. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 22(1), 75- 92. Ciborra, C. U. (1993). Teams, Markets and Systems: Business Innovation and lnfonnation Technology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Ciborra, C. U., 8 Patriotta, G. (1996). Groupware and teamwork in new product development: The case of a consumer goods multinational team. In C. U. Ciborra (Ed.), Groupware and Teamwork (pp. 121-142). New York: John Wiley 8 Sons Ltd. Cohen, J., 8 Cohen, P. (1983). Applied Multiple Regression / Correlation Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Cohen, S. G., 8 Bailey, D. E. (1997). What makes teams work: Group effectiveness research from the shop floor to the executive suite. Journal of Management, 23(3), 239-290. Cohen, S. G., 8 Mankin, D. (2002). Complex collaborations in the new global economy. Organizational Dynamics, 31(2), 117-133. Cohen, W. M., 8 Levinthal, D. A. (1990). Absorptive capacity: A new perspective on Ieaming and innovation. Administrative Science Quarterty, 35, 128-152. Coleman, J. S. (1988). Social capital in the creation of human capital. American Joumal of Sociology, 94(Supplement), 95-120. Cool, C., Fish, R. S., Kraut, R. E., 8 Lowery, C. M. (1992). Iterative design of video communication systems. CSCW '92. November. Toronto. Cramton, C. (1997). Information problems in dispersed teams. In L. Dosier 8 J. Keys (Eds.), Academy of Management Best Paper Proceedings (pp. 298- 302). Georgia: Southern University. 100 Cummings, T. G. (1978). Self-regulating work groups: A socio—technical analysis. Academy of Management Review, 3, 625-634. Davidow, W. H., 8 Malone, M. S. (1992). The Virtual Corporation. New York: Harper-Collins. DeSanctis, G., 8 Monge, P. (1999). Introduction to the special issue: Communication processes for virtual organizations. Organization Science, 10(6), 693-703. Donellon, A., Gray, 8., 8 Bougon, M. G. (1986). Communication, meaning, and organized action. Administrative Science Quarterly, 31 ( 1 ), 43-55. Dourish, P., 8 Bellotti, V. (1992). Awareness and coordination in shared workspace. CSCW '92. November. Toronto, Canada. Dourish, P., 8 Bly, S. (1992). Portholes: Supporting awareness in a distributed work group. CSCW '92. November. Toronto, Canada. Druskat, V. U., 8 Wolff, S. B. (2001). Building the emotional intelligence of groups. Harvard Business Review, 80(3), 81-91. Fandt, P. M. (1991). The relationship of accountability and interdependent behavior to enhancing team consequences. Group and Organization Studies, 16, 300-312. Feldman, M. S. (1989). Order Without Design: Information Production and Policy Making. Stanford. CA: Stanford University Press. Fussell, S. R., Kraut, R. E., Lerch, F. J., Scherlis, W. L., McNally, M. M., 8 Cadiz, J. J. (1998). Coordination, overload and team performance: Effects of team communication strategies. CSCW '98. Seattle; Gabarro, J. J. (1978). The development of trust, influence and expectations. In A. G. Athos 8 J. J. Gabarro (Eds.), Interpersonal Behaviors: Communication and Understanding in Relationships (pp. 290-303). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Galbraith, J. (1973). Designing Complex Organizations. Reading, MA: Addison- Wesley. Geen, R. G. (1991). Social motivation. Annual Review of Psychology. 42, 377- 399. 101 Gioia, D. A. (1986). Symbols, scripts, and sensemaking Creating meaning in the organizational experience. In The Thinking Organization (pp. 49—74). San Francisco, CA: Jossey—Bass. Gladstein, D. L. (1984). Groups in context: A model of task group effectiveness. Administrative Science Quarterly, 29(4), 499-517. Goodman, P. S., Ravlin, E. C., 8 Argote, L. (1986). Current thinking about groups: Setting the stage for new ideas. In P. S. Goodman (Ed.), Designing Effective Work Group. San Francisco: Jossey—Bass. Granstrand, 0., Bohlin, E., Oskarsson, C., 8 Sjoberg, N. (1992). External technology acquisition in large multi-technology corporations. Research and Development Management, 22(2), 111-133. Gresov, C. (1989). Exploring fit and misfit with multiple contingencies. Administrative Science Quarterly, 34(3), 431—452. Gutwin, C., 8 Greenberg, S. (1997). Workspace Awareness. CHI'97 Workshop on Awareness in Collaborative Systems. March 22-27, 1997. Atlanta, Georgia. Gutwin, C., Roseman, M., 8 Greenberg, S. (1996). A usability study of awareness widgets in a shared workspace groupware system. CSCW '96. November. Cambridge, Mass. Guzzo, R. A., 8 Shea, G. P. (1992). Group performance and intergroup relations in organizations. In M. D. Dunnette 8 L. M. Hough (Eds.), Handbook of Industrial and Organizational Psychology (pp. 269-313). Palo Alto, CA: Psychological Press. Hackman, J. R. (1987). The design of work team. In J. W. Lorsch (Ed.), Handbook of Organizational Behavior (pp. 315-342). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. Hackman, J. R., 8 Morris, C. G. (1975). Group tasks, group interaction process, and group performance effectiveness: A review and proposed integration. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 8, pp. 45-99). New York: Academic Press. Hagedoom, J. (1993). Understanding the rationale of strategic technological partnering: lnterorganizational modes of cooperation and sectoral differences. Strategic Management Journal, 14, 371-385. 102 Harkins, S., 8 Petty, R. (1982). Effects of task difficulty and task uncertainty on social loafing. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43, 1214- 1229. Harper, R. H. R., 8 Hughes, J. A. (1993). "What a f-ing system! Send ’em all to the same place and then expect us to stop The Hitting”: Making technology work in air traffic control. In G. Button (Ed.), Technology in Working Order: Studies of Work, Interaction and Technology (pp. 127- 144). Routledge, UK: Taylor 8 Francis Books Ltd. Harper, R. H. R., Hughes, J. A., 8 Shapiro, D. Z. (1989). Working in harmony: An examination of computer technology in air traffic control. ECSCW’89: the , First European Conference on Computer Supported Cooperative Work,. September 13—15, 1989. Gatwick, London. It. Heath, C., 8 Luff, P. (1992). Collaboration and control: Crisis management and multimedia technology in London Underground Line control rooms'. Computer Supported Cooperative Work, 1 (1 ), 69-95. Heath, C., Svensson, M. 8., Hindmarsh, J., Luff, P., 8 Vom Lehn, D. (2002). Configuring awareness. Computer Supported Cooperative Work, 11, 317— 347. Heath, C. C., 8 Luff, P. (1991). Collaborative activity and technological design: Task coordination in London Underground control rooms. ECSCW’91: the Second EurOpean Conference on Computer-Supported Cooperative Work. September 24—27, 1991. Amsterdam. Hightower, R., 8 Hagmann, C. (1995). Social influence effects on remote group interactions. Journal of lntemational Information Management, 4(2), 17-32. Hirst, M. K. (1988). Intrinsic motivation as influenced by task interdependence and goal setting. Journal of Applied Psychology, 73, 96-101. Hollingshead, A. B., McGrath, J. E., 8 O’Connor, K. M. (1993). Group task performance and communication technology: A longitudinal study of computer-mediated versus face-to-face work groups. Small Group Research, 24(3), 307-333. Huddleston, S., Doody, S. G., 8 Ruder, M. K. (1985). The effect of prior knowledge of the social Ioafing phenomenon on performance in a group. lntemational Journal of Sport Psychology, 16, 176-182. Hughes, J., King, V., Rodden, T., 8 Andersen, H. (1994). Moving Out from the Control Room: Ethnography in System Design. Conference on Computer- Supported Cooperative Work. October 22-26. Chapel Hill, NC, USA,. 103 Hughes, J. A., Randall, D., 8 Shapiro, D. (1992). Faltering from ethnography to design. CSCW '92. November. Toronto, Canada. Hughes, J. A., Randall, D., 8 Shapiro, D. (1993). From ethnographic record to system design: Some experiences from the field. Computer Supported Cooperative Work, 1(3), 123-141. Huysman, M., Steinfield, C., Jang, C., David, K., Huis in 't Veld, M., 8 Poot, J., Mulder, I. (forthcoming). Virtual Teams and the Appropriation of Communication Technology: Exploring the Concept of Media Stickiness. Journal of CSCW, To be published in 2004. Hwang, P., 8 Burgers, W. P. (1997). Properties of trust: An analytical view. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 69(1), 67-73. Jaccard, J., 8 Turrisi, R. (2003). Interaction Effects in Multiple Regression (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications. Jang, C.-Y., Steinfleld, C., 8 Pfaff. B. (2000). Supporting awareness among virtual teams in a web-based collaborative system: the TeamSCOPE system. CSCW'OO Awareness Workshop. December, 2000. Philadelphia, PA. Jang, C.-Y., Steinfield, C., 8 Pfaff. B. (2002). Virtual team awareness and groupware support: an evaluation of the TeamSCOPE system. lntemational Journal of Human-Computer Interaction, 56(1), 109-126. Jarvenpaa, S. L., 8 Ives, B. (1994). The Global Network Organization of the Future: Information Management Opportunities and Challenges. Journal of Management Information Systems, 10(4), 25-57. Jarvenpaa, S. L., Knoll, K., 8 Leidner, D. E. (1998). Is anybody out there? Antecedents of trust in global virtual teams. Journal of Management Information Systems, 14(4), 29-64. Jarvenpaa, S. L., 8 Leidner, D. E. (1998). Communication and Trust in Global Virtual Teams. Journal of Computer Mediated Communication, 3(4), Retrieved February 20, 2002, from http://www.ascursc.(lg/icmc/volflmlissue2004/iarvenpaa.html. Johnson, J. J. (1999). A field study of partially distributed group support. 32nd Hawaii lntemational Conference on System Sciences. January, 1999. Hawaii. 104 Johnson-George, C. E., 8 Swap, W. C. (1982). Measurement of specific interpersonal trust: Construction and validation of a scale to assess trust in a specific other. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43, 1306- 1317. Kahai, S. 8., 8 Cooper, R. B. (1999). The effect of computer-mediated communication on agreement and acceptance. Journal of Management Information Systems, 16(1), 165-188. Kandel, E., 8 Lazear, E. P. (1992). Peer pressure and partnerships. Journal of Political Economy, 100(4), 810-817. Karolak, D. W. (1998). Global Software Development: Managing Virtual Teams and Environments. Los Alamitos, CA: IEEE Computer Society Press. Kelly, J. R., 8 McGrath, J. E. (1985). Effects of time limits and task types on task performance and interaction of four-person groups. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49, 395-406. Kerr, N. (1983). Motivation losses in small groups: A social dilemma analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 819-828. Kerr, N. L., 8 Brnun, S. E. (1981 ). Ringelmann revisited: alternative explanations for the social Ioafing effect. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 7, 224-231. Kerr, N. L., 8 Bruun, S. E. (1983). Dispensability of member effort and group motivation losses: Free-rider effects. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 78-94. Kiggundu, M. N. (1983). Task interdependence and job design: Test of a theory. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 31, 145-172. Kiggundu, M. N. (1983). Task interdependence and job design: Test of a theory. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 31, 145-172. Knoll, K., 8 Jarvenpaa, S. L. (1998). Working together in global virtual teams. In M. Igbaria 8 M. Tan (Eds.), The Virtual Workplace (pp. 2-23). Hershey, PA, USA: Idea Group Publishing. Kristof, A. L., Brown, K. G., Henry P. Sims, J., 8 Smith, K. A. (1995). The virtual team: A case study and inductive model. In M. M. Beyerlein, D. A. Johnson 8 S. T. Beyerlein (Eds.), Advances in Interdisciplinary Studies of Work Teams (Vol. 2, pp. 229-253). Greenwich, Conn.: JAI Press. 105 Kumar, M, Scheer, L. K., 8 Steenkamp, J.-B. E. M. (1995). The effects of perceived interdependence on dealer attitudes. JMR, Journal of Marketing Research, 32(3), 348-356. Lam, S. S. K., 8 Schaubroeck, J. (2000). Improving group decisions by better pooling information: A comparative advantage of group decision support systems. Joumal of Applied Psychology. 85(4), 565-586. Langfred, C. W. (2000). Work-group design and autonomy: a field study of the interaction between task interdependence and group autonomy. Small Group Research, 31 (1 ), 54-70. Latane, B., Williams, K., 8 Harkins, S. (1979). Many hands make light the work: The causes and consequences of social Ioafing. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 822-832. Lee, E.-J., 8 Nass, C. (2002). Experimental tests of normative group influence and representation effects in computer-mediated communication: When interacting via computers differs from interacting with computers. Human Communication Research, 28, 349-381. Lewicki, R. J., 8 Bunker, B. B. (1996). Developing and maintaining trust in working relationships. In R. M. Kramer 8 T. R. Tyler (Eds.), Trust in Organizations: Frontiers of Theory and Research (pp. 114-139). London: Sage. Lewin, K. (1951). Field Theory in Social Science. New York: Harper and Bros. Lewis, J. D., 8 Weigert, A. (1985). Trust as a social reality. Social Forces, 63, 967-985. Liden, R. C., Wayne, S. J., 8 Bradway, L. K. (1997). Task interdependence as a moderator of the relation between group control and performance. Human Relations, 50(2), 169-181 . Lindenberg, S. (1994). Norms and the power of loss: Ellickson's theory and beyond. Journal of Institutional and Theoretical Economics, 150(1), 101- 1 13. Lindenberg, S. (1997). Grounding groups in theory: Functional, cognitive, and structural interdependencies. In B. Markovsky, M. J. Lovaglia 8 L. Troyer (Eds.), Advances in Group Processes (Vol. 14). Greenwich, Connecticut: JAI Press Inc. Lipnack, J., 8 Stamps, J. (2000). Virtual Teams: People Working Across Boundaries Wrth Technology. New York: Wiley. 106 Lucas, H. C. J., 8 Baroudi, J. (1994). The role of information technology in organization design. Journal of Management Information Systems, 10(4), 9-23. Luff, P., Heath, C., 8 Greatbatch, D. (1992). Tasks-in-interaction: paper and screen based documentation in collaborative activity. Computer-supported Cooperative Work. December, 1992. Toronto, Ontario, Canada. Maier, N. R. G. (1963). Problem-Solving Discussions and Conferences. New York: McGraw-Hill. March, J. G., 8 Simon, H. A. (1958). Organizations. New York: John Wiley 8 Sons, Inc. Markus, M. (1990). Toward a "critical mass" theory of interactive media. In J. Fulk 8 C. Steinfield (Eds.), Organizations and Communication Technology (pp. 194-218). Newbury Park, CA: Sage Publication. Matsuura, N., Fujino, G., Okada, K.-I., 8 Matsushita, Y. (1996). Venus: A tele- communication environment to support awareness for informal interaction. In D. Shapiro, M. Tauber 8 R. Traunmuller (Eds.), The Design of Computer Supported Cooperative Work and Groupware Systems (pp. 227-237). Amsterdam: Elsevier Science B. V. Mayer, R. C., Davis, J. H., 8 Schoorman, F. D. (1995). An integration model of organizational trust. The Academy of Management Review, 20(3), 709- 731. Maznevski, M. L., 8 Chudoba, K. M. (2000). Bridging space over time: virtual team dynamics and effectiveness. Organization Science, 11(5), 473-492. McAllister, D. J. (1995). Affect- and cognition-based trust as foundations for interpersonal cooperation in organizations. Academy of Management Journal, 38, 24-59. McGrath, J. E. (1964). Social Psychology: A Brief Introduction. New York: Holt. McGrath, J. E., 8 Hollingshead, A. (1993). Putting the "group" back in group support systems: Some theoretical issues about dynamic processes in groups with technological enhancements. In L. Jessup 8 J. Valacich (Eds.), Group Support Systems: New Perspectives (pp. 78-96). New York: Macmillan. Medina-Mora, R., Winograd, T., Flores, R., 8 Flores, F. (1992). The action workflow approach to workflow management technology. CSCW '92. November. Toronto, Canada. 107 Mintzberg, H. (1979). The Structuring of Organizations. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. Monge, P., 8 Fulk, J. (1999). Communication technology for global network organizations. In G. DeSanctis 8 J. Fulk (Eds.), Shaping Organization Form: Communication, Connection, and Community (pp. 71 -1 00). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publication. Morris, 8. A., Marshall, T. E., 8 Rainer, R. K. J. (2002). Impact of user satisfaction and trust on virtual team members. Information Resources Management Journal, 15(2), 22-30. Mowshowitz, A. (1997). Virtual organization. Communications of the ACM, 40(9), 30-37. Olson, J. S., 8 Teasley, S. (1996). Groupware in the wild: Lessons learned from a year of virtual collocation. CSCW '96. November. Cambridge, MA. Orlikowski, W. J., 8 Gash, D. C. (1994). Technological frames: Making sense of information technology in organizations. ACM Transactions on lnfonnation Systems, 12(2), 174-207. Palmer, J. W., 8 Speier, C. (1998). Teams: Virtualness and media choice. lntemational Journal of Electronic Commerce, 3(1), 27-48. Pauleen, D. J. (2003). Lessons learned crossing boundaries in an lCT-supported distributed team. Journal of Global Information Management, 11(4), 1-19. Pearce, J. A. |., 8 Ravlin, E. C. (1987). The design and activation of self- regulating work groups. Human Relations, 40(11), 751-760. Philip, H., 8 Dunphy, D. (1959). Developmental trends in small groups. Sociometry, 22, 162-174. Price, K. H. (1987). Decision responsibility, task responsibility, identifiability, and social loafing. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 40, 330-345. Prinz, W., Rodden, T., Syri, A., 8 Trevor, J. (1996). Modeling cooperative work settings with active workspaces. In D. Shapiro, M. Tauber 8 R. Traunmuller (Eds.), The Design of Computer Supported Cooperative Work and Groupware System (Vol. 12, pp. 195-226). Amsterdam, the Netherlands: Elsevier Science B. V. Putnam, R. (2000). Bowling Along: The Collapse and Revival of American Community. New York: Simon 8 Schuster. 108 Rempel, J. K., Holmes, J. G., 8 Zanna, M. D. (1985). Trust in close relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49, 95-1 12. Rutkowski, A. F., Vogel, D. R., van Genuchten, M., Bemelmans, T. M. A., 8 Favier, M. (2002). E-collaboration: The reality of virtuality. IEEE Transactions on Professional Communication, 45(4), 219-230. Saavedra, R. P., Earley, P. C., 8 Van Dyne, L. (1993). Complex interdependence in task-performing groups. Journal of Applied Psychology, 78, 61-72. Salas, E., Dickinson, T. L., Converse, S. A., 8 Tannenbaum, S. l. (1992). Toward an understanding of team performance and training. In R. W. Swezey 8 E. Salas (Eds.), Teams: Their training and performance (pp. 3-29). Norvvood, NJ: ABLEX. Sandor, O., Bogdan, C., 8 J., B. (1997). Aether: An Awareness Engine for CSCW. The Fifth European Conference on Computer-Supported Cooperative Work. September 7—1 1. Lancaster, UK. Sarker, S., Valacich, J. S., 8 Sarker, S. (2003). Virtual team trust: Instrument development and validation in an IS educational environment. lnfonnation Resources Management Journal, 16(2), 35-55. Schmidt, K. (2002). The problem with ‘awareness’. Computer Supported Cooperative Work, 11 , 285-298. Schoorman, F. D., Mayer, R. C., 8 Davis, J. H. (1996). Empowerment in veterinary clinics: the role of trust in delegation. Working Paper. Department of Organizational Behavior and Human Resource Management, Purdue University. Shaw, M. E. (1976). Group Dynamics: The Psychology of Small Group Behavior (2nd ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill, Inc. Shaw, M. E., 8 Briscoe, M. E. (1966). Group size and effectiveness in solving tasks varying in degree of cooperation requirements: University of Florida. Shea, G. P., 8 Guzzo, R. A. (1987). Groups as human resources. Research in Personnel and Human Resource Management, 5, 323-356. Siegel, J., Dubrovsky, V., Kiesler, S., 8 McGuire, T. W. (1986). Group processes in computer-mediated communication. Organizational Behavior and Human Processes, 37, 157-187. Simon, H. A. (1969). The sciences of the artificial. Cambridge: M.I.T. Press. 109 Sitkin, S. B., 8 Roth, N. L. (1993). Explaining the limited effectiveness of legalistic "remedies" for trust/distrust. Organization Science, 3(4), 367-392. Souder, W. E. (1975). Achieving organizational consensus with respect to R&D project selection criteria. Management Science, 21, 669-681. Starbuck, W. H. (1992). Learning by knowledge-intensive firms. Journal of Management Studies, 29(6), 713-740. Steinfield, C., Goodman, E., Lloyd, J., David, K., 8 Hinds, T. (1998). Globally distributed engineering design teams: Cultural, social, and technological factors influencing group process and performance: NSF proposal. Steinfield, C., Huysman, M., David, K., Jang, C., Poot, J., Huis in't Veld, M., et al. (2001). New methods for studying global virtual teams: Towards a multi- faceted approach. Hawaii lnternational Conference On System Sciences. Maui, Hawaii. Steinfield, C., Jang, C.-Y., 8 Pfaff, B. (1999). Supporting virtual team collaboration: the TeamSCOPE system. Group'99. Nov. 11-14. Phoenix, AZ. Straus, S. G. (1999). Testing a typology of tasks: An empirical validation of McGrath's (1984) group task circumplex. Small Group Research, 30(2), 166-187. Straus, S. G., 8 McGrath, J. E. (1994). Does the medium matter? The interaction of task type and technology on group performance and member reactions. Journal of Applied Psychology, 79(1), 87-97. Suchan, J., 8 Hayzak, G. (2001). The communication characteristics of virtual teams: A case study. IEEE Transactions on Professional Communication, 41(3), 174-186. Swain, A. (1996). Social loafing and identifiability: The mediating role of achievement goal orientations. Research Quarteriy for Exercise and Sport, 67(3), 337-344. Szymanski, K., 8 Harkins, S. G. (1987). Social loafing and self-evaluation with a social standard. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53(5), 891- 897. 110 Tannenbaum, S. I., Beard, R. L., 8 Salas, E. (1992). Team building and its influence on team effectiveness: An examination of conceptual and empirical developments. In K. Kelley (Ed.), Issues, Theory, and Research in Industrial / Organizational Psychology (pp. 117-153). Amsterdam, Holland: Elsevier. Tollmar, K., Sandor, O., 8 Schomer, A. (1996). Supporting social awareness @Work: Design and experience. CSCW '96. November. Cambridge, Mass. Tollmar, K., 8 Sundblad, Y. (1995). The design and building of the graphic user interface for the collaborative desktop. Computers 8 Graphics, 19(2), 179- 188. Townsend, A. M., DeMarie, S. M., 8 Hendrickson, A. R. (1998). Virtual teams: Technology and the workplace of the future. Academy of Management Executive, 12(3), 17-29. Tubbs, S. L. (1983). A Systems Approach To Small Group Interaction (2nd ed.). Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley Publishing. Van de Ven, A., Delbecq, A., 8 Koenig, R. (1976). Determinants of coordination modes within organizations. American Sociological Review, 41, 322-338. Van de Ven, A. H., Delbecq, A. L., 8 Koenig, R. J. (1976). Determinants of coordination modes within organizations. American Sociological Review, 41(3), 322-338. Van der Vegt, G., Emans, B., 8 Vande Vliert, E. (1999). Effects of lnterdependencies in project teams. The Joumal of Social Psychology, 139(2), 202-214. Vegt, G. V. d., Emans, B., 8 Vliert, E. V. d. (1999). Effects of interdependencies in project teams. The Journal of Social Psychology, 139(2), 202-214. von Eye, A., 8 Schuster, C. (1998). Regression Analysis for Social Sciences. San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Wageman, R. (1995). Interdependence and group effectiveness. Administrative Science Quarteriy, 40, 145-180. Wall, T. D., Kemp, N. J., Jackson, P. R., 8 Clegg, C. W. (1986). Outcomes of autonomous work groups: A long-term field experiment. Academy of Management Journal, 29, 281 -304. 111 Weick, K. (1979). The Social Psychology of Organization (2nd ed.). New York: Random House. Weick, K. E. (1995). Sensemaking in Organization. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications. Wigand, R., Picot, A., 8 Reichwald, R. (1997). lnfonnation, Organization and Management: Expanding Markets and Corporate Boundaries. Chichester, UK: John Wiley 8 Sons Ltd. Williams, K., Harkins, S., 8 Latane, B. (1981). Identifiability as a deterrent to social loaflng: Two cheering experiments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 40(2), 303-311. Williams, K. D., Nida, S. A., Baca, L. D., 8 Latane, B. (1989). Social loafing and swimming: Effects of identifiability on individual and relay performance of intercollegiate swimmers. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 10, 73-81. Wong, C. S., 8 Campion, M. A. (1991). Development and test of a task level model of motivational job design. Journal of Applied Psychology, 76, 825- 844. Yoo, Y., 8 Alavi, M. (2001). Media and group cohesion: Relative influences on social presence, task participation, and group consensus. MIS Quarteriy, 25(3), 371 -390. Zirger, B., 8 Maidique, M. (1990). Empirical testing of a predictive model of new product development. Management Science, 36(7), 51-60. Zucker, L. G. (1986). Production of trust: Institutional sources of economic structure, 1840-1920. In B. M. Staw 8 L. L. Cummings (Eds.), Research in organizational behavior (Vol. 8, pp. 53-111). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. 112 I,lllllllIIIlllIl‘l‘llllllllllllll llllll‘lll’ll 3 1293 02504 9291