(NWWIWIHHWIWNIINHIHJHMH(”MINIMUM 1 5 3 3 (D\| C; J Q‘ Lu This is to certify that the dissertation entitled THE EFFECT OF RACE AND EXPERTISE 0N SOURCE CREDIBILITY RATINGS presented by Angela Celeste Farr has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for Ph.D. degree in communication Date May 5 . 2003 MS U is an Affirmative Action/Equal Opportunity Institution Mot/w Major professor 0-12771 PLACE IN RETURN BOX to remove this checkout from your record. To AVOID FINES return on or before date due. MAY BE RECALLED with earlier due date if requested. DATE DUE DATE DUE DATE DUE .: r) :7 .. I 6/01 cJCIRC/DaieDue.p65~p. 15 THE EFFECT OF RACE AND EXPERTISE ON SOURCE CREDIBILITY RATINGS By Angela Celeste Farr A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Communication Arts and Sciences 2003 ABSTRACT By Angela Celeste Farr Source credibility affects our daily interpersonal interactions in various ways yet our understanding of credibility is still limited. Expertise is one of the most often listed factors of credibility and until recently, the definition and measurement construct has received little attention by persuasion scholars. Also, it has been suggested that race may play a role in credibility ratings. This study assessed the relative influence of race and expertise on credibility judgments. The sample consisted of 403 white undergraduate students at a large Midwestern university. Technical and practical competencies were found to be independent constructs, each with significant effects on credibility ratings. When both competencies are high credibility ratings will be maximized. Race had no effect on the judgments of competency or credibility. Limitations of the study and implications for future research were discussed. Copyright by ANGELA CELESTE F AR 2003 DEDICATION I gratefully dedicate this dissertation to Ametta Cage and Dr. Lawrence Redd, who helped me begin this process, my family who loved and supported me before and throughout this process, to my committee for their guidance, and my GOD who granted me the fulfillment of a life-long dream. iv ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS I would like to express my appreciation to my chair, Dr. Kim Witte for her faith, devotion, and commitment to my success. You are responsible for a great deal of my growth. I would also like to thank Dr. Mary Bresnahan for her support, understanding, and encouragement. You helped me become an outstanding teacher through following in your footsteps. Dr. Robbie Steward, you are amazing. You have guided me through one of the most challenging experiences in my life, and you have done so with grace, patience, and everlasting encouragement. You helped me maintain my sanity, and I will always be grateful. Last, but not least, I would like to thank Dr. Franklin Boster. You introduced me to the tools that I would require for success. You had faith in my talents when others did not. You were willing to defend the “underdog” when so many others were incorrect in their assumptions. Thank you for your guidance, openness, and consistent honesty. I will be forever grateful to each of you for your contributions to my academic success. TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF FIGURES ................................................................................. vi INTRODUCTION .................................................................................. 1 METHODS ......................................................................................... 12 RESULTS ........................................................................................... l 6 DISCUSSION ...................................................................................... 24 APPENDIX A ....................................................................................... 28 APPENDIX B ....................................................................................... 33 APPENCIX C ...................................................................................... 35 APPENDIX D ...................................................................................... 36 REFERENCES .................................................................................... 37 vi LIST OF FIGURES Survey Instrument (APPENDIX A) ............................................................... 28 Confirrnatory Factor Analysis for Credibility, Practical and Technical Competencies (APPENDIX B) ...................................................................................... 33 Correlation Matrices (APPENDIX C) .......................................................... 35 Pr0posed Path Model (APPENDIX D) 36 vii INTRODUCTION Womce Credibility Source credibility affects our daily interpersonal interactions, decision-making, the diffusion of innovations, and our judgment of the persuasiveness of speakers with their various messages. We observe the importance of a credible source as we discuss the effectiveness of politicians, product spokespersons, salespersons, and a multitude of other ‘persuasive’ individuals. Scholars believe that receivers’ schemas about different groups of people like males/ females, blacks/whites, gays/straights, etc., all influence judgments of source credibility, yet despite over 50 years of research on credibility, our understanding of the construct and its effects on related constructs is limited. One limitation of our understanding of source credibility is the belief that we fully understand the construct. Some scholars believe that as a field, our “intense level of inquiry has apparently fostered the belief that few questions regarding the persuasive effects of source credibility remain unanswered. As a result, interest in this topic has waned” (Stemthal, Phillips, & Dholakia, 1978). McCrosky and Young (1981) contributed to the belief that scholars have definitively defined credibility; a belief made apparent as they stated, ‘er believe it is time, once and for all, to call a halt to the proliferation of factor analytic studies of source credibility; that is, those intended to ‘discover’ its dimensions” (p. 34). They concluded by suggesting “factor analysis must be preceded by careful conceptualization and construct delineation, or the product of even the most massive research effort. ..will lead to nothing, or worse, to inappropriate knowledge claims” (McCrosky & Young, 1981). So despite the fact that the literature is filled with contrasting claims, various factors of source credibility, and unclear effects, some scholars believe that source credibility is a fully explicated construct. A second limitation, which follows from the first, is the lack of consistent definition of the construct. Hovland and his colleagues (1953) defined source credibility as the combination of “the extent to which a communicator is perceived to be a source of valid assertions” and “the degree of confidence in the communicator’s intent to communicate the assertions he considers most valid” (p.21). Markham (1968), following in the footsteps of Anderson and Clevenger (1961), suggested that source credibility is simply Aristotle’s construct of ethos. Aristotle defined ethos as a “listener’s evaluation of a speaker’s intelligence, character, and good will (p. 361)” (Delia, 1976). Berlo and colleagues (1969-70) chose not to utilize the term ‘source credibility’ because they believed that the variable would be viewed as a property of the source rather than the receiver. To alleviate this problem, they created “dimensions for evaluating message sources” and utilized Hovland and associates’ definition of credibility as the foundation for their work. McCroskey and Young (1981) defined the construct as “the attitude toward a source of communication held at a given time by the receiver” (p. 24). Many scholars simply neglect to define the term in their research, assuming that readers have a working definition of the construct or will consistently identify a ‘credible’ source. The final limitation in our understanding of source credibility is found in the factor structure of the construct itself. Generally, scholars are unsure how many factors compose the variable “source credibility,” what names those factors should have, and how those factors should be measured. For example, most scholars suggest that both trustworthiness and expertise are aspects of credibility (Applbaum, & Anatol, 1973; Chaiken, & Maheswaren, 1994; Hovland, Janis, & Kelly, 1953; Posner, & Kouzes, I988; Sobczak, & Bowers, 1993; Stemthal, Phillips, & Dholakia, 1978; Tuppen, 1974; Wu, & Shaffer, 1987). However, some scholars accept trustworthiness as a key factor contributing to credibility judgments (Heyman, 1992; Hovland, & Weiss, 1951; Markham, 1968; Powell, & Wanzenried, 1995) while others maintain that expertise is a key factor (Giffin, 1967; King, 1976; Nosek, Fuhrer, & Hughes, 1991; Swenson, Nash, & Roos, 1984). Other scholars select different naming schemes leading to remaining factors that are often cited for influencing judgments of credibility, such as reliability (Chaiken, & Maheswaren, 1994; Wu, & Shaffer, 1987), dynamism (Posner, & Kouzes, 1988), competence, character, sociability, composure, and extroversion (McCrosky, & J enson, 1975; Powell, Wanzenried, 1995). One consistently suggested component of credibility which until recently, has received little attention within the persuasion literature, is expertise. While the trustworthiness and expertise as dimensions of source credibility judgments are fiequently noted, the conceptualization and definition of the construct, ‘expertise,’ have just begun to be examined in depth. Recently, Farr (2003) argued that expertise can be perceived in two distinct ways: technical competence and practical competence. This dissertation will fitrther examine judgments of expertise (type of competence), and how expertise, together with race, influence perceived source credibility. The following is a brief review of the expertise literature leading to a discussion of the potential effects of race. 1‘) Role of Perceived Expertise Cognitive psychologists have long sought to define expertise and determine how it is acquired. Typically, they focus on expertise as an intrinsic characteristic, which suggests that expertise is an inherent feature of the source. In pursuit of defining the characteristics of expertise, three overlapping yet distinct perspectives of expertise have emerged in the cognitive psychology literature. The first perspective of expertise, developed by Ericsson and Smith (1991), focuses on distinguishing outstanding versus non-outstanding individuals in a domain. Outstanding individuals are determined by outstanding performance within a given domain or sphere of expertise. Exemplars of outstanding performance are faster response times for tasks in the domain, superior ability to plan ahead, superior memory performance, highly developed perceptual/attentive abilities, a strong sense of what is relevant, an ability to simplify complex problems, ability to efi‘ectively communicate their expertise, ability to handle adversity, selectivity in choosing decision problems, appearing outwardly confident, and having an extensive and up-to-date content knowledge (Ericsson & Smith, 1991; Shanteau, 1988). The second expertise perspective focuses on an individual’s accumulation of knowledge, with special emphasis on the type of knowledge held. Rosenberg (1997) states that “expertise results from amassing both great amounts of knowledge and the ability to perform pattern-based retrieval during many years of experience in a specific area” (p.37). In the third perspective, expertise is determined by factors such as training, ability (Bimbaum & Stegner, 1979; Cusella, 1982; Hovland et. al, 1953; McGuire, 1969) nomination by peers, experience, and hands-on experience (Perez, Johnson, & Emery, 1995). In 1964, F itts proposed a model containing three stages of expertise development through practice of tasks within a given domain. The first stage is the “cognitive stage” and is “characterized by an effort to understand the task, its demands, and to learn what information one must attend” (Ericsson & Smith, 1991). The second stage is the “associative stage” which involves “making the cognitive process efficient to allow rapid retrieval and perception of required information” (Ericsson & Smith, 1991). The final stage is the “autonomous stage” in which “performance is automatic and conscious cognition is minimal” (Ericsson & Smith, 1991). In summary, these three cognitive psychology perspectives of expertise have several common factors. Each perspective suggests that experts are knowledgeable, have extensive experience, and are outstanding in their domain of expertise. In contrast to the cognitive psychology perspective, when studying expertise from the persuasion literature, the focus is on the perceived expertise of a message sender, rather than on the internal characteristics related to expertise. In this literature, expertise is often manipulated without being defined or without verification of the success of the manipulation on receivers. For example, some scholars will manipulate level of training, experience, role status, attraction and the presence or absence of a diploma (Swenson, Nash, & Roos, 1984) as a representation of expertise level. In a study by Nosek, Fuhrer, and Hughes (1991), expertise was manipulated by stating that one set of counselors had doctoral degrees and professional practices in counseling, whereas the contrasting group of counselors had jobs of an unspecified nature and volunteered their services as counselors at an independent living center. Norman (1976) portrayed the expert source as a professor of physiological psychology at a large Canadian university who had recently co-authored a book. His low expert counterpart was portrayed as an undergraduate in a general arts program at a large Canadian university (Norman, 1976). Another expertise manipulation described an individual with 15 years of experience in a field, publications of articles in the field, as well as maintaining a private practice as an expert while the low expertise condition described an individual who has worked one year after recently completing his/her degree and is beginning to establish his/her research career (Swenson, et al., 1984). In each of these studies, the condition with the “expert” resulted in greater perceived credibility as compared to the condition labeled “non-expert.” Yet, the construct “expertise” was never formally defined, just manipulated. In both the cognitive psychology literature and the persuasion literature, expertise seems to have two major characteristics, the first being, practical competence and the second, technical competence. For example, in the cognitive psychological literature, experts were thought to have in-depth knowledge, training, and experience. Similarly, in the persuasion literature, those labeled experts were those who either had symbols (e.g., academic degrees) or experience (e.g., experience in a field). It appears that all of these expertise definitions or manipulations fall into two categories — practical competence and technical competence. Each construct is briefly defined below. Technical Competence Technical competence is skillfulness by virtue of possessing special knowledge. It is achieved through training, reading, formal education, and scholarly pursuits. A technically competent expert will often be perceived as being on a different social, power, or authority level than those with whom they interact. When manipulating the technical competence aspect of expertise, many scholars manipulate the level of degree earned, social status, training, formal education, and title which usually places the expert on a different social, power, or authority level than those by whom they are rated. The focus on the manipulation of technical competence suggests that technical competence leads to perceived expertise. Practical Competence In contrast, practical competence, or experienced expertise, is developed through direct observation or participation in an event. For the purposes of this study, practical competence will be defined as skills that result from direct participation in events or activities. This type of competence is developed through practice, experience, or trial and error. The practically competent individual does not necessarily understand the body of literature, mechanical workings, or expert language of the field, but is an expert primarily through their experiences in the field. Their practical experience makes others perceive them as an expert. Research on Competence. Farr (2003) demonstrated that both technical and practical competencies are relevant to the measurement of perceived expertise. When practical competence was low, technical competence had little effect, but when practical competence was high, the addition of technical competence greatly increased perceived credibility. This study sought to replicate this finding by manipulating both technical and practical competencies, to reaffirm their impact on credibility ratings, as well as examine the influence of race on perceptions of expertise. Role of Race Affirmative Action has long been a topic for debate. Currently, the nation is again considering the question of race as a factor in decision making for university entrance and other hiring considerations. If race is in fact, a non-issue, as some claim, then scholars should be able to observe the lack of a relationship between race and credibility. Surprisingly, the variable race has received only cursory attention in the source credibility literature. Therefore, one purpose of this dissertation is to examine the effects of race on judgments of source credibility. In today’s increasingly diverse world, it is important to understand the influence that race might have on perceived credibility. If a relationship is observed between race and credibility, the explanation for why this may be the case will be a question which remains outside of the scope of the current work. For this purpose of this paper, however, the question is simply, does race make a difference on ratings of source credibility? Following is a brief review of the effects of race on various outcomes. The Effects of Race. Despite affinnative action and other civil rights laws, the destruction of J irn Crow and other segregationist practices, and the growing acceptance of diversity, some believe that race still matters in people’s judgments of others’ credibility, qualifications, etc. (West, 1993). Despite the belief that race matters, scholars have not addressed the general question of how race affects credibility ratings on a daily basis in larger society. Some scholars, being aware that evaluations are important for tenure and job retention, have addressed the specific question of race and instructor credibility. Hendrix (1998) found that students believed that it would be more difficult for a Black professor to establish credibility in the classroom, especially when they were teaching courses unrelated to their race. The students also believed that Black professors have to work harder to perform successfully within the academic system. Conversely, Patton (1999) found that African American instructors were more credible than European instructors. While studying credibility of a salesperson from the perspective of a consumer, scholars found that the race of the consumer will affect the credibility rating of the salesperson, especially related to likeability (Jones, Moore, Stanaland, & Wyatt, 1998). White consumers felt that the salespeople portrayed in the study were more likeable than did Black consumers. From the perception of consumers, the Black salespersons were considered more likable, most trustworthy, most attractive, and highest on expertise, with perceptions of white females following, and white males rated lowest (Jones, et al., 1998). Although this finding sounds promising on the face of it, the authors (Jones, et al., 1998) contend that these results might not indicate a boost in the credibility of Black or women, but rather represent a discounting or backlash against the White male salesperson who may have primed negative stereotypes against the “typical“ salesperson (i.e. pushy, aggressive, and forceful). When attempting to diversify a sales force, credibility may not be the strongest issue, but race can cause the hiring and retention process to become a challenge due to discriminatory practices. Several scholars have found that in the 1980s and 19908, black men in the United States were suffering a 12 to 15 percent loss in earnings due to labor market discrimination (Darity, Guilkey, & Winfrey, 1996; Darity, & Mason, 1998; Gottschalk, 1997; Rodgers, & Spriggs, 1996). Others have found that having a black racial identity and a darker skin tone will reduce an individual’s chances of working by 52 percent after controlling for education, age, and criminal record (Johnson, Beinenstock, & Stoloff, 1995; Darity, & Mason, 1998). It has also been reported that sales managers tend to “screen out” African American and female applicants to select instead equally qualified White male applicants (Jolson, 1983). In contrast, a slightly more recent study was unable to observe discrimination in hiring (Marshall, Stamps, & Moore, 1998). When Afiican Americans are hired, they often still face challenges in excess to those required by the job. African Americans reported experiencing exclusion by White colleagues in networking activities such as golf outings at “restricted” country clubs (Comer, Nicholls, and Vermillion, 1998; Thomas and Wetlaufer, 1997). Also, African Americans have had problems working with their customer base that range fiom “awkwardness in initial person-to person meetings (after telephone contacts), to loss of the sale, to overt messages that ‘Black salespeople are not welcome’” (Comer, Nicholls, and Vermillion, 1998; Lucas, 1996). Essentially, the role of race on perceptions of credibility has yet to be determined. Some scholars suggested that race affects credibility ratings whereas, others suggest that race does not matter. The goal of this paper is to determine if race affects credibility ratings. Summa_ry This study seeks to extend the credibility literature by examining the effects of individual race on perceived credibility and expertise ratings, although this study will be limited to two races-- African Americans and Whites. If race does not affect people’s perceptions, then we would expect to see no significant difference between the credibility and/or expertise ratings due to race manipulations. lO As we consider the perceived credibility ratings of both African Americans and Whites, one component of expertise that may vary between the groups is perceived eXpertise. Whites have greater numbers of college educated individuals as well as more individuals reaching higher levels of education, which suggest that Whites should have an expertise advantage. The real question of interest is not if Whites have greater expertise, but when expertise is controlled, will they receive higher ratings of perceived expertise, and subsequently higher credibility ratings? 11 Method Design A 3 (Black, White, and no portrayal of race) x 2 (technical competence; high versus low) x 2 (practical competence; high versus low) factorial design was employed, with credibility ratings as the dependent variable. Participants were randomly assigned to one of the 12 conditions in the 3 x 2 x 2 design specified above. Participants A total of 403 students in communication courses at Michigan State University were recruited for the study and received extra credit for their participation, resulting in an average of 34 participants per cell. This sample size is adequate to detect differences between cells, if they in fact exist, according to conventional standards of power (beta = .80, alpha = .05, one-tailed, based on a .30 expected effect size). Given the limited number of minority participants and the irnprobability of equal participation across cells, all participants were White and were 36% male (N = 145), and 64% female (N = 258). Given the student’s status as undergraduates, the mean age (M = 21.1, SD = 2.3) is younger than that of the general population, although it is fairly representative of the traditional undergraduate population which usually has an age range between 18 - 22 years old. Procedure The participants in the study entered the meeting room and were welcomed to the research project by the research assistant. At this time they completed the informed consent form. Upon receipt of the completed informed consent form, the research 12 assistant gave each participant a folder. The folder contained the manipulation and randomly assigned the participants to one of the twelve groups. On the outside of the folder was a fabricated job posting for a communication consultant which lists the criteria required, and skills necessary for the position. Within the folder was the race manipulation (photograph or not), and a technically competent (high vs. low) or practically competent (high vs. low) resume. Each participant was asked to complete a survey relating to the attributes of each applicant and to make the decision to offer the portrayed individual an interview (or not). Once the survey was completed, the participants were thanked, debriefed and dismissed from the study. Wm Induction of Race. Students were randomly assigned to one of three groups. One folder contained a picture of a Black female, while another folder contained a folder of a White female, and the final folder contained no picture. All three photograph combinations were matched with an expertise manipulation (a resume). The individuals in the photographs wore white blouses, had similar makeup, jewelry, hair length, height, weight, and build so that each could be presented as equally attractive. Both photographs were head shots with the confederates smiling gently while looking directly into the camera. They were photographed against a white background to eliminate identifying features or cues of any kind. For ease of matching the general characteristics of the individuals, women were selected. Although no differences are expected based upon gender, future studies could replicate this study with men to ensure that gender does not affect the manipulations. Induction of Technical Competence. Technical competence is based upon the accumulation of knowledge through scholarly pursuits, reading, or training, so in the high 13 technical competence condition, the participant received the resume of a recently graduated Ph.D. in Communication. The work experience category of this resume held only one year at McDonalds, a position which she held during high school and is not relevant to the position she is currently seeking. She was presented as having received her bachelors degree in Communication from Illinois State University, her MBA from Northwestern University, and her doctorate degree from Illinois State University in Communication, and was seeking her first professional work experience with the Success Consulting Group in Chicago. In the low technical competence condition, the applicant has earned a BA. in Communication from Illinois State University with the same lack of work experience presented previously. Induction of Practical Competence. Practical competence is based upon the idea that experience is necessary for expertise, so to manipulate high practical expertise, the participant received the resume of a high school graduate who documented ten years of work experience with a (fictional) consulting agency in Chicago. The resume presented a woman who began working with a consulting agency in her high school work-study program. Upon graduation from high school, she accepted a job within that agency as a part-time Consultant’s Assistant, then received a promotion to full-time Consultant’s Assistant, with her most recent promotion making her the Consultant’s Assistant to one of the highest ranking consultants in the agency. She listed several speaking engagements. She too, was seeking her first full consultant position with the Success Consulting Group in Chicago. In the low practical competence condition, the same high 14 school graduation listed 4 years of work experience rather than ten years, with all other aspects of the resume remaining the same. Pilot Tests. The manipulations were piloted and refined until the high and low technically competent resumes received significantly different ratings on the technical competence scale and the practically competent resumes received significantly different ratings on the practical competence scale (high versus low). A confirmatory factor analysis was run on the data to determine if there is in fact a three factor solution which represents credibility, practical competence, and technical competence. Specifically, confirmatory factor analysis procedures outlined by Hunter and Gerbing (1982) were used to test first-order and second-order unidirnensionality of the scales and constructs. The scales were internally consistent and achieved parallelism (APPENDIX B). Manipulation Checks P_racticaLCompetence. After being exposed to the induction, four items were used to assess the participant’s perception of the practical competence of the source. The participants were asked to rate on a 5 item semantic differential the following items: practiced, realistic, experienced, and participant. The items were selected fiom items previously used within the credibility literature and which through face validity measured practical competence. These items were then averaged to create a mean score. The standardized item alpha for this scale was .84. An independent subjects t test was performed to insure that the variable had been manipulated as expected. A significant main effect for practical competence was found 15 on the practical competence measure, 1(1, 15) = 2.17, p<.05. This finding suggests that practical competence was manipulated such that those reading the high practical competence message believed the speaker to be more practically competent (M = 3.94, SD = .59) than those reading the low practical competence message (M = 2.97, SD = 1.12). As a confound check, we tested the practical competence manipulation against the technical competence scale and it was non-significant [t (1, 15) = .55, p>.05, n.s.], suggesting a clean manipulation for practical competence. Technicpl Competence After being exposed to the induction, three items were used to assess the participant’s perception of the technical competence of the source. The participants were asked to rate on a 5 item semantic differential the following items: educated, competent, and intellectual. The items were selected from items previously used within the credibility literature and which through face validity measured technical competence. The items were then averaged to create a mean score. The standardized item alpha for this scale was .83. An independent subjects t test was performed to ensure that the variable had been manipulated as expected. A significant main effect for technical competence was found on the technical competence measure, 10, 15) = 2.19, p<.05. This finding suggests that technical competence was manipulated such that those reading the high technically competence message believed the speaker to be more technically competent CM = 4.22 SD = .62) than those reading the low technically competent message (M = 3. 36, SD = .84). 16 As a confound check, we tested the technical competence manipulation against the practical competence scale and it was non-sigrrificant[t (1, 15) = -.64, p>.05, n.s.], suggesting a clean manipulation for the technical competence manipulation. Outcome Measures (Dependent Variables) Credibilig; To measure credibility, 10 items from Ohanian’s (1990) source credibility semantic differential scale were used. The scale which Ohanian (1990) developed was well constructed, well tested, and reported high reliability coefficients. Consistent with the high reliability coefficients reported by Ohanian (1990), when utilizing the scale to address the current data, the scale received an standardized item alpha of .78. The items used measure the attractiveness and trustworthiness factors of source credibility. The items utilized to measure attractiveness were “attractive-unattractive, classy- not classy, beautiful-ugly, elegant-plain, [and] sexy-not sexy” (Ohanian, 1990). The items selected to measure trustworthiness were “dependable-undependable, honest- dishonest, reliable-unreliable, sincere-insincere, [and] trustworthy-untrustworthy” (Ohanian, 1990). All items were mixed into a series of five-point semantic differentials from which a credibility score was developed. The obtained scores for all of the items were averaged to create a credibility rating for each participant. This single score credibility rating was then utilized as a data point for the AN OVA analysis. 17 Results Credibility ratings were analyzed with a technical competence (high vs. low) x practical competence (high vs. low) x race (white, black, no picture) between subjects factorial analysis of variance. An alpha level of .05 was used for all statistical tests. As noted in the discussion about manipulation checks, both practical and technical competencies were manipulated cleanly in the pilot test. Another way to verify the strength of the manipulation is to note the strong uncorrected correlation between the manipulation of practical competence and high ratings on the practical competence scale (r = .51). Likewise, there is a strong uncorrected correlation between the manipulation of technical competence and high ratings on the technical competence scale (r = .71). Given that both technical and practical competencies are measuring expertise, it is expected, and noted that we observed a strong uncorrected correlation between the competencies (r = .64). Also, the uncorrected correlation from the practical competence scale to the credibility measure was strong (r = .76) as was the uncorrected correlation from the technical competence scale to the credibility measure (r = .81) (APPENDIX C). Correcting the correlations for error in measurement, the correlations increased in strength such that the correlation between the manipulation of practical competence and high ratings on the practical competence scale (r = .56) is stronger. Likewise, with the correlation between the manipulation of technical competence and high ratings on the technical competence scale (r = .78). The correlation from the practical competence scale to the credibility measure was decreased (r = .4) as did the corrected correlation from the technical competence scale to the credibility measure (r = .36) (APPENDIX C). Overall, 18 using the practical and technical competence measures, credibility scores can be predicted with significant accuracy, F (1, 4) = 301.53, p < .01, R = .87 (APPENDIX C). From this position, a path model has been proposed (APPENDIX D) which fits the data well. As practical competency was increased, scores on the practical competency scale also increased as noted through the path coefficient ([3 = .56). As technical competency was increased, scores on the technical competency scale also increased as noted through the path coefficient (B = .78). As predicted, both practical and technical competencies affect credibility ratings, as evidenced by the path between the practical competence scale and credibility ratings (B = .41) and the technical competence scale and credibility ratings ([3 = .69). As credibility ratings increase, people are more likely to recommend that the candidate receive an interview (r = .82). Given the predictive power of all aspects of the model, we submit this model for further consideration and testing. An independent subjects t test was performed to insure that the variables had been manipulated as independently. A significant main effect for practical competence was found on the practical competence measure, I: (1, 399) = 169.21, p<.01, n2 =.27. We then tested the practical competence manipulation against the technical competence scale which was also significant 13 (1, 399) = 67.00, p<.01, n2 =.l 1. A significant main effect for technical competence was found on the technical competence measure, E (1, 399) = 455.82, p<.01, n2 =.52. We then tested the technical competence manipulation against the practical competence scale which was also significant F (1, 399) = 29.29, p<.01, n2 =.03, suggesting that the manipulation of one variable impacts the manipulation of the other. 19 Results also showed a significant main effects for technical competence on credibility, E (1, 391) = 161.83, p<.01, n2 =.29 and significant main effects for practical competence on credibility were found, E ( 1, 391) = 66.99, p<.01, 112 =.15. There was not a main effect for race F (1,2) = .13, p>.05, n2 =.001, n.s., nor were any of these main effects affected by interaction effects between race and practical competence, _F_‘ (2,391) = .99, p>.05, n2 =.005, n.s., race and technical competence, F (2,391) = .329, p>.05, n2 =.002, n.s., practical and technical competence, E (1,391) = .17, p>.05, 112 =.005, n.s., or the combination of race, practical and technical competence, _If‘_ (2,391) = 1.03, p>.05, n2 =.005, n.s. Overall, credibility is a good predictor of the recommendation to interview the applicant (R = .73). The technical and practical competence scales also functioned well as predictors of the recommendation to interview (R = .77), but having all three measures increased predictive power to its maximum (R = .7 8). Significant main effects for credibility were observed on recommendations to interview the applicant, E (1, 17) = 12.87, p<.Ol, n2 =.39. Significant main effects were also observed for manipulations of practical competence on recommendations to interview the applicant, E (1, 349) = 20.72, p<.01, n2 =.06. Significant main effects were observed for manipulations of technical competence on recommendations to interview the applicant, E (l, 349) = 4.3, p<.05, n2 =0]. These main effects were not affected by interaction effects between credibility and the technical competence manipulation, F (1,12) = 1.52, p>.05, n.s., nor the credibility, manipulation of technical, and practical competence combination, F (1,7) = 1.02, p<.05, n.s. There was an interaction between credibility and the manipulation of practical competence on recommendation to interview the applicant, E (1, 14) = 2.04, p<.05, n2 =.08, such that as 20 practical competence increased, the recommendation to interview the applicant also increased. When examining the cell means related to the significant main effects for practical and technical competence, we observe that when both practical and technical competence are presented as high, students rate the speaker more credible (M = 4.15, SD = .51) as compared to the low technical and low practical competence speaker (M = 2.99, SD = .55) d = 1.67. Alternatively, when practical competence was high and technical competence was presented as low, students rated the speaker as slightly less credible (M = 3.52, SD = .56) than the high technical and low practical competence speaker (M = 3.77, SD = .60) d = .36. In sum, the higher the perceived competence (both practical and technical), the stronger the credibility ratings. However, when isolating each competency, perceived technical competence produces stronger credibility ratings than perceived practical competence. Race, being manipulated through the presentation of a picture of a Black woman, a White woman, or no picture, did not play a significant role on measurements of credibility. The participants were asked to report ‘from memory’ the race of the ‘applicant’ after they completed their credibility ratings. As expected, in conditions where a picture accompanied the resume, the participants were almost perfect in their report of race, when participant were reporting the race of the pictured white applicant three of the 132 participants reported incorrectly, and when reporting the race of the pictured Black applicant, seven of the 132 participants reported incorrectly. All ten of the participants who answered incorrectly failed to respond to the item, or wrote that they could not remember the race of the applicant. In the control (no picture) condition, 95 of 21 the 132 participants answered the item incorrectly, while 37 answered correctly. All 95 of the participants who responded incorrectly reported that the applicant was White, while the 37 who gave correct responses selected other, skipped the item, or wrote that they did not know the answer. This suggests that the majority of the participants assumed that the non—pictured applicant was a white candidate. Despite the assumption, the role of race on measurements of credibility was nil. To increase the internal consistency of the credibility measure, the participants were asked to assess how strongly they believed that the applicant was an expert consultant, and if the applicant had the necessary experience and the necessary education to be a good consultant. Each of these items were strongly correlated with credibility such that the stronger the belief of expertise (r = .71), experience (r= .67), and education (I = .70) the higher credibility ratings the applicant received. Not only were these items strongly correlated with credibility, they were also strongly correlated with each other. The stronger the beliefs about experience (r = .76) and education (I =.65), the greater the belief that the applicant was an expert consultant. Also, the more the applicant was believed to have the education necessary to be a good consultant(r = .55), the greater the belief that the applicant also had the necessary experience to be a good consultant. Using these three items to predict credibility ratings would allow for good a prediction (R = .80), but not as effective as the technical and practical competency scales (R = .87). Also, as ratings of credibility increased, the applicants were more likely to recommend that the applicant receive an interview (r = .73). In sum, the more experience and education an applicant is perceived to have, the greater their expertise and credibility 22 ratings. This also suggests that as credibility ratings increase, the likelihood of being recommended for an interview will also increase. 23 Discussion The assertion that technical and practical competencies have independent effects on credibility ratings was consistent with the data, although the hypothesis that when practical competence was low, technical competence had little effect, but when practical competence was high, the addition of technical competence greatly increased perceived credibility was unsupported by the data. These finding are consistent with the conceptualization of expertise as being domain specific. In certain domains, it is more important to have technical competency to be perceived as an expert, whereas in other domains the reverse is true. The fact remains that those who are both high in technical and practical competencies will be perceived as the most expert and credible. This is also consistent with the literature’s portrayal of an expert as being someone who is an outstanding individual within a domain as determined by outstanding performance within a given domain or sphere of expertise(Ericsson & Smith, 1991), who has both great amounts of knowledge and the ability to perform pattern-based retrieval during many years of experience in a specific area (Rosenberg, 1997), who is well trained, has great ability (Bimbaum & Stegner, 1979; Cusella, 1982; Hovland et. a1, 1953; McGuire, 1969) is nominated or respected by peers, and experienced (Perez, Johnson, & Emery, 1995). We can conclude that across domains, the higher one is in both competencies, the more credible they will be perceived. This increase in credibility also leads to increased desirability, as observed by the increasing recommendation to interview a candidate based upon their perceived credibility. These findings suggest that academic understanding alone is not sufficient to create expertise; one must also have practical experiences to support and balance the education that is received, a finding which is particularly important for those in training or educational roles. It is not enough to have a student population which can regurgitate theories, or pass standardized tests. It is also 24 imperative that students have Opportunities to become experienced within their field which will increase their ability to practically apply the material they have leamed to new and different situations. Although it is unreasonable to assume that a novice student will progress to the autonomous stage of processing (F itts, 1964) through a brief period of practical application, it is plausible that the novice could progress from the cognitive stage through their educational experiences into the associative stage through the integration of their experiences with their understanding. Creating programs which include both academic and practical components will lead to a more expert student population, which in turn will increase the credibility of both the student and the institution, and ultimately increase the employment rates of graduates which can also increase the credibility of the institution from which these experts were developed. Because expertise is domain specific, it is important that we determine within which domains technical competence may be of greater import than practical competence. As scholars create methods to determine within which domains technical competency is more important and within which domains practical competency is of greater importance, training programs and educational institutions will be able to use their research to focus on the most appropriate competency for their domain, while understanding that both competencies are imperative, and produce experts who are the strongest possible representation of their field of study. The understanding that both technical and practical competencies are necessarily to perceptions of expertise should also be salient to budding scholars and new instructors. As we present new material to our student population, it is not enough to break down theories into digestible bites, then expect the students to present their understanding on exams or through papers. We must also incorporate an experiential aspect to the leaming environment, both through the presentation of our own experiences, and through projects which will require the students to have experiences related to the theories they have studied. As instructor, if you are able to explain the intricate workings of a theory, yet 25 are unable to give a personal example of the theory in action, given the findings of this study, your credibility will be reduced with your students. Although a class cannot be built upon personal antecedent alone, personal antecedents can highlight the fact that the instructor is practically competent in the subject matter they are teaching, which should lead to higher ratings of credibility. This increased credibility should ultimately increase student evaluations, which is a goal for which we all strive. Another aspect of this study, the effects of race on perceived credibility, produced hopefiil results. The effect of race on expertise and credibility was not significant. Although this resonates within some of the literature such as Patton’s (1999) finding that African American instructors were more credible than European instructors, many would find this result strange. This study was limited by the lack of racial diversity among participants. As previously noted, the world is quite diverse and becoming increasingly so by the day. This study did not account for the voices of the minority population due to limited access at the large Midwestern University. So future research should seek minority participants and compare their responses to those presented in this work. Also, this study limited race to black and white ‘applicants’ only. The work could be extended by adding other races and comparing those findings to the ones reported in this work. Although this study has limitations, as all studies do, the limitations do not explain the nature of the results observed. The undergraduates sampled in this study did not alter their credibility and expertise ratings using race as a guide, so perhaps, as they become the decision makers of tomorrow, they will continue to assess the qualification of candidates without emphasis being placed upon their race. Despite the fact that this is a hopeful result for the future, it is also a non-representative result given the current population of decision makers. Undergraduates, generally seem to be more liberal in their views as Opposed to the larger society. Undergraduates also tend to be idealistic in their views, due to their lack of experience. For future research, it would be intriguing to replicate this study on a non-undergraduate sample. We should hold on to the hope that 26 the current undergraduate population is equitable in their decision making practices, but a test within the current decision making population would better assess the current state of the use of race in hiring decisions. Although race was not a factor determining the credibility or expertise of the hypothetical job applicant, there was an interesting, yet unwarranted assumption that was frequently reported. The assumption that an applicant without a photograph was white was prevalent, yet unwarranted. Future research could seek to understand the cognitive processes underlying the assumption, what caused peOple to assume that the applicant was white? It is possible that the education and experiences presented, as well as the name “Jennifer Johnson” were consistent with the participants’ experiences of White applicants. Through the utilization of the growing body of literature relating to expectancy violations, future research could consider the question of how racially related expectancy violations may change (or not) credibility ratings. Through this study, we can be more confident that technical and practical competencies are independent constructs, but when both competencies are high credibility ratings will be at their highest. This study is consistent with the previous explication and measurement of practical and technical competence as components of expertise with technical competence being skillfulness by virtue of possessing special knowledge which is achieved through training, reading, formal education, and scholarly pursuits and practical competence, or experienced expertise being skills that result fi'om direct participation in events or activities which is developed through practice, experience, or trial and error. The literature has long supported the notion that expertise is a component of credibility (Applbaum, & Anatol, 1973; Chaiken, & Maheswaren, 1994; Hovland, Janis, & Kelly, 1953; Posner, & Kouzes, 1988; Sobczak, & Bowers, 1993; Stemthal, Phillips, & Dholakia, 1978; Tuppen, 1974; Wu, & Shaffer, 1987). We can conclude that expertise, when well measured, is a strong predictor and necessary factor of credibility and that credibility can influence the desirability an applicant. 27 APPENDIX A Survey Instrument Thank you for your feedback toward our applicant. All feedback that we receive is important, and we value your input. Before you begin the survey, please list everything that you remember about the applicant, and your impressions of how his/her skills will meet the needs of our organization. 28 Our applicant would like to have your advice concerning the best way to create a resume. What information would you include, change, or delete from the resume that you were presented? What should the applicant do to enhance his/her resume? 29 Please circle the response_which best matches your feelings about the Applicant. Classy Trained Biased Educated Elegant Realistic Qualified Skilled Informed Able Sexy Intelligent Objective Reliable Beautiful Honest Expert Believable Competent Sincere Credible Intellectual Empathic Practiced Dependable Participant Attractive Trustworthy Experienced Authoritative Understanding Knowledgeable mmmmmmmmmmmmummmmmmmmmmmmmmmmmmu. J:Ahhhhhhb-Ahhhbhhh-hhhhAAA-bhhbhhA4:- wwwwwwwwwwwuwwuwwwwwwwuwuwwwwwbjw 30 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 l HHD—‘p—t c—Ip—A '. F“ # p—a y—fl p—O 5—. 0—. F. D—5 0—. —5 I-I‘ i—i 0—. I=I y—e H H H Not Classy Untrained Unbiased Uneducated Plain Ideahsfic Unqualified Unskilled Uninforrned Inept Not Sexy Unintelligent Subjective Unreliable Ugly, Dishonest Inexpert Unbelievable Incompetent Insincere Not Credible Narrow Not Empathic Beginning Undependable Observer Unatlractive Untrustworthy Inexperienced Not Authoritative Judgmental Not Knowledgeable Please answer each of these questions about the applicant based upon their resume which was provided and our job description. 1. I would recommend that this applicant receive an interview by members of your organization. Strongly agree Moderately agree Barely agree Disagree Disagree strongly 2. I believe that this applicant is an expert consultant. Strongly agree Moderately agree Barely agree Disagree Disagree strongly 3. I believe that this applicant has the experience necessary to be a good consultant. Strongly agree Moderately agree Barely agree Disagree Disagree strongly 4. I believe that this applicant has the education necessary to be a good consultant. Strongly agree Moderately agree Barely agree Disagree Disagree strongly 5. I believe that this applicant has the skills necessary to be a good consultant. Strongly agree Moderately agree Barely agree Disagree Disagree strongly 6. I believe this applicant has presented presentations as a consultant. Strongly agree Moderately agree Barely agree Disagree Disagree strongly 7. I believe this applicant has had consulting experiences. Strongly agree Moderately agree Barely agree Disagree Disagree strongly 8. I would be more likely to recommend someone who is highly educated instead of someone who is highly experienced. Strongly agree Moderately agree Barely agree Disagree Disagree strongly 9. I believe that natural talent is more important than educational experience. Strongly agree Moderately agree Barely agree Disagree Disagree strongly 10. I believe that experience and education are equally important. Strongly agree Moderately agree Barely agree Disagree Disagree strongly 11. I believe that I would like this applicant. Strongly agree Moderately agree Barely agree Disagree Disagree strongly 12. I believe that I am similar to this applicant. Strongly agree Moderately agree Barely agree Disagree Disagree strongly 31 Without referrin to the a lication materials lease answer the following questions. What was the ethnicity of today’s applicant (please circle one)? Caucasian Afiican-American Hispanic Asian Multi-ethnic Other What was the approximate age of the applicant? What was the sex (male/female) of the applicant? What was the name of the applicant? How much education did the applicant have? How much work experience did the applicant have? Were there any mistakes in the resume document? Yes No If yes, how many mistakes did you find? Please tell us a little about ourself. Remember all of our res onses will remain confidential. 1. What is your sex? Male Female 2. What is your age? 3. What is your ethnicity (please circle one)? Caucasian African-American Hispanic Asian Multi-ethnic Other 32 APPENDIX B Confirmatory Factor Analysis for Credibility, Practical and Technical Competencies .60 .000 .000 0 L0 90 .000 :0 00.9 3.0 —00.0 2.9 00.0 .80 .0. .00 F0 .000 .0 2.9.20. «.0. 0.0. «0.0. .000. 000. 0.0. .000. .2 .000 .0v0i—000 00.0 000 00 00.? ~00 .000 00.0. 000 00.0. .0. F000 00.0 .000 30 8.9 6.0 00.0 .30 3.0 00.0. 0 .0 W00 00 000 60 00.0- 3.0. .000 5.9 000. 00.0 .0 .000 000 000 00.0 00.0 a B0 000. 0 .0 .000 000 00 00.9 .000 8.9 :0. 00.0. .0 .30 00 0.00 .000. 000 .00 0.0 .0 .50 00.0 3.0 .000 .0 00.0 .0 W00 .000 0.0 . 0.0.0. 00. .0 E —00 00.0 . 2.0. .N .20 .000 .000 . .0 g .000 .80 N. Z 0. .0 0 n 0 0 0 .0 0 p P 5.322 .otm % .00 .000 .50 $0 .000 0.0 00.0 0.0 000 0.0 ~00 00.0 «.0 .00.0 .0. E .000 .00.0 .000 000 8.0 00.0 000 00.0 00.0 00.0 .00 .00.0 .5 00.0 .000 .00.0 0.0 v0 00 000 00.0 8.0 «0.0 00.0 T00 .0. «00 000 .000 «0.0 R0 00.0 00 00.0 000 0.0 .00 .0 .Mm 8.0 8H0 0.0 3.0 00.0 000 9.0 00.0 3.0 —0 . 00.0 «A0 00.0 000 00.0 .000 0.0 .00 .0. .WM WM M000 30 000 .000 3.0 .010 "m E 000 :0 .000 .00 3.0 3.0 . .000 «0.0 «00 7 E .000 00 . 00.0 000 .0 .E .00 .000 _ 0.0 .m “w "~00 .R0 . .P 000 «00 .00 u _ P 2 S 0 0 a 0 0 v .m u 0 . 8.2020 r 05.0 mmd 5d 5d @5d de mmd and «dd 30.0 5.0 mod m5d 55d ndm m5d F Nod mwd m5d mvd mwd 5©d mmd mmd Pmd mNd mmd wd N5d Now wmd mad 9 5.0 Nwd mod M5d mod N5d 5.0 5nd 50.0 5.0 m5d 55d an 5.0 mod 5d mmd v0.0 mmd 5vd mvd mvd mvd 5nd mNd m5d Nod Nod up 5.0 m5d Nwd v0.0 mvd mmd 5md vmd mmd mmd $20 mmd 5.0 mod Gd : 05d mvd mod med mmd 5md Nvd wmd de mvd Nd Nvd 55d mmd 0d Op de mmd m5d 5vd 5md Nvd M5d mmd m5d vmd mmd Ed 3.0 50.0 0.0 m and 5®d mmd mvd vmd 5d and mvd omd md mmd NNd vvd mmd mvd d mmd mmd N5.d mvd wmd Nod m5d mmd m5d Fwd wmd NNd wvd 00.0 0.0 5 No.0 mod 5d mvd mmd mvd vmd md Fwd Nvd Nvd mud and wmd vmd o 5.0 .00 00 000 S0 00 00.0 00.0 00.0 000 00.0 .00 .000 0.00 00.0 0 5.0 mNd 3d mNd find Nrd de NNd NNd mwd .bd 5rd Nd NNd vwd v dwd mod 5d m5d 5.0 55.0 Ed 3d mvd mvd de Nd mvd Nod v0.0 m $5.0 md $5.0 Nod mod and 50d mmd mod mmd 5nd NNd Nod «0.0 55.0 N E0 ~50 2.0 00.0 00 0.0 0.0 000 0.0 00.0 000 00.0 «00 K0 0.0 w 000 «00 50 a. 3 0. 0 0 0. 0 0. 0 0 w _ 530:. 02.0.0003 0200. .000 ES. 000 00.0 00. 0.. 00 088080. 50.0 0.0.0 05 8. mm anBoEn. «0.0 050 00 00 00. £36.06 “000.020 ”00:90 0.0002005 00.60.an 3.56000 0.5.3 00252000 00.00... 00.0.0000 000.03. 0.. Z 0. u aeoocoww 0 5 u 088.0% 0 m m u 32.9006 “030:0. 00 0.200. 0. 0009000 0000 0>0c 0E0: 34 APPENDIX C Correlation Matrices . 0..“ ....... .0 E... 8 . 0 . . «a 0 9.3.08". . an o 3 o. and Ned. A2000» 0:30:08 .0 0 30 S0. 80 .283 058880 a 0 No.0. 28:05:20.5 9:00:03 H p €0=£a§cu§ 05000080 3333023. $.33 .233 «co—00.30.58. Esau-assoc. . _ 30.28:. g 3.5.09.0 9:02.02 150802: 9:00:08 080800.... 0. 5.3: 5:22.60 08850 F 20 00.0 2.0 .00 9.0 06.33... w «0.0 05.0 0.0 Nnd 350—320 F 3.0 2.0 2.0 A2300 0:80:08 . .00 50 58». 05888.. .. wed. Eczsascafi. 0:80:03 P €323.22... 053.00.... $.33 . .2000. E23395...» E0:n.:&:a.5 3230.:— a=3=080 0:50:02 050208.. 9:00:02 0500000.... _ 5382 2626—2500 —fl:—Oto 35 APPENDIX D [ Practical Competence Manlgflatlon ‘ \ / 7 Technical Competence Manipulation Proposed Path Model Practical Competence Scale l | B=.62 l Technical Competence Scale 36 .41 .69 r=.82 Interview References Anderson, K., & Clevenger, Jr., T. (1963). A summary of eXperimental research in ethos. Speech Monographs, 30, 59-78. Applbaum, R. L., & Anatol, K. W. E. (1973). Dimensions of source credibility: A test for reproducibility. Speech Monographs. 40. 231-237. Berlo, D. K., Lemert, J. B., & Mertz, R. J. (Winter, 1969-1970). Dimensions for evaluating the acceptability of message sources. Public Qpinion Quarterly, 33 (4), 363-376. Bimbaum, M. H., & Stegner, S. E. (1979). Source credibility in social judgment: Bias, expertise, and the judge’s point of view. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, (1), 48-74. Chaiken, S., & Maheswaran, D., (1994). Heuristic procession can bias systematic processing: Effects of source credibility, argument ambiguity, and task importance on attitude judgment. J ournflf Personality and Social Psychology. gg, (3), 460-473. Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum. Comer, L. B. Nicholls, J. A. F. &Vermillion, L. J. (1998). Diversity inthe sales force: Problems and challenges. Journal of Personal Selling and Sales Management, 18, (4),1-20. Cusella, L. P (1982). The effects of source expertise and feedback valence on intrinsic motivation. Human Qommunication Research, 9, (1), 17-32. Darity, W. Jr., Guilkey, D., & Winfrey, W. (1996). Explaining differences in economic performance among racial and ethnic groups in the USA: The data examined. American Journal of Economics and Sociology, 55, (4), 411-26. 37 Darity, W. Jr., & Mason, P. L. (1998). Evidence on discrimination in employments: Codes of color, codes of gender. The Journal of Economic Perspectives, 12, (2), 63-90. Delia, J. G. (1976). A constructivist analysis of the concept of credibility. Quarterly Jouanl of Speech. 62. 361-375. Ericsson, K. A., & Smith, J. (1991). Prospects and limits of the empirical study of expertise: An introduction. In Ericsson, K. A., & Smith, J. (Eds), Toward a general theog of expertise: Prospects and limits (pp. 1-38). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Farr, A. C., & Witte, K. (2003). Defining expertise to create a more effective conceptualization of source credibility. Manuscript under review. Fitts, P. M. (1964). Perceptual-motor skill learning. In A. W. Melton (Ed.), Categories of human learning (pp. 243-285). New York: Academic Press. Giffin, K. (1967). The contribution of studies of source credibility to a theory of interpersonal trust in the communication process. Psychological Bulletin, 68, (2 ), 104-1 19. Gottschalk, P. (1997). Inequality, income growth and mobility: The basic facts. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 11 (2), 21-40. Hendrix, K. G. (1998). Student perceptions of the influence of race on professor credibility. Journal of Black Studies, 28, (6), 738-763. Heyman, S. (1992). A study of Australian and Singaporean perceptions of source credibility. Communication Research Reports, 9, (2), 137-150. Hovland, C. I, & Weiss, W. (1951-1952). The influence of source credibility on communication effectiveness. Public Qpinion Qparterly, 15,I (4), 635-650. Hovland, C. I., Janis, I. L., & Kelley, H. H. (1953). Communication and persuasion: Psychological studies of opinion change. New Haven: Yale University Press. 38 Johnson, Jr., J. H., Beinenstock, E. J ., & Stoloff, J. A. (1995). An empirical test of the cultural capital hypothesis. The Review of Black Political Economy, 23, (4), 7- 27. Jolson, M. A. (1983). Employment barriers in selling: Are Blacks and Females welcome? In Proceedings of the American Marketing Association, Patrick E. Murphy eta1., Eds. Chicago: American Marketing Association, 267-270. Jones, E., Moore, J. N., Stanaland, A. J. S., & Wyatt, R. A. J. (1998). Salesperson race and gender and the access and legitimacy paradigm: Does difference make a difference? Journal of Personal Selling and Sales Management, 18, (4), 71-78. King, S. W. (1976). Reconstruction the concept of source perceptions: Toward a paradigm of source apprOpriateness. Western Speech Communication, 216-225. Lucas, A. (1996). Race matters. Sales and Marketing Management, 148, 50-62. Markham, D. (1968). The dimensions of source credibility of television newscasters. Ih_e Journal of Communication 18 57-64. Marshall, G. W., Stamps, M. B., & Moore, J. N. (1998). Preinterview biases: The impact of race, physical attractiveness, and sales job type on preinterview impressions of sales job applicants. J ournjl of Personal Selling and Sales Management, 18, (4), 21-38. McCroskey, J. C., & Young, T. J. (1981). Ethos and Credibility: The construct and its measurement after three decades. Central States Speech Journal, 32, 24-34. McCroskey, J. C., & J enson, T. A. (1975). Image of mass media news sources. Journal of Broadcasting, 19, (2), 169-180. McGuire, W. J. (1969). The nature of attitudes and attitude change: In G. Linzey & Aronson (Eds), The handbook of spcial psychology (v.3; pp 352-379). Reading: Addison-Wesley. 39 Norman, W. T. (1963). Toward an adequate taxonomy of personality attributes: replicated factor structure in peer nomination personality ratings. Journal of abnormal and social psychology, 66, (June), 574-583. Nosek, M. A., Fuhrer, M. J ., & Hughes, S. O. (1991). Perceived counselor credibility by persons with physical disability: Influence of counselor disability status, professional status, and the counseling content. Rehabilitation Psychology, 36, (3), 153-161. Ohanian, R. (1990). Construction and validations of a scale to measure celebrity endorsers’ perceived expertise, trustworthiness, and attractiveness. Journal of Advertising, 19, (3), 39-52. Patton, T. O. (1999). Ethnicity and gender: An examination of its impact on instructor credibility in the university classroom. The Howard Journal of Communications _1_0, 123-144. Rodgers III, W., Spriggs, W. B., and Waaler, E. (1996). What does AF QT really measure: Race, wages, schooling and the AF QT score. The Review of Black Political Economy, 24, (4), 13-46. Perez, R. S., Johnson, J. F., & Emery, C. D. (1995). Instructional design expertise: A cognitive model of design. Instructional Science 23 321-349. Posner, B. Z., & Kouzes, J. M. (1988). Relating leadership and credibility. Psychological Rgports, 63, 527-530. Powell, F. C., & Wanzenried, J. W. (1991). Perceptual changes in source credibility: Repeated test using two candidates during a political campaign. W Motor Skills 73, 1107-1114. Rosenberg, J. I. (1997). Expertise research and clinical practice: A suicide assessment and intervention training model. Educational Psychology Review, 9, (3), 279- 296. 40 Shanteau, J. (1988). Psychological Characteristics and strategies of expert decision makers. Acta Psychologica, 68, 203-215. Sobczak, P. M., & Bowers, M. R. (1993). An experimental design for measuring the effects of source credibility and goal behavior estimate in health promotion television commercials. Psychology, A Journal of Human Behavior. 30. (2), 30- 43. Stemthal, B., Phillips, L. W., & Dholakia, R. (1978). The persuasive effect of source credibility: A situational analysis. Public Qpinion Qparterly, 42, (3), 285-314. Swenson, R. A., Nash, D. L., & Roos, D. C. (1984). Source credibility and perceived expertness of testimony in a simulated child-custody case. Professional PsychologLResearch and Practice 15 (6) 891-898. Thomas, D. A., & Wetlaufer, S. (1997). A question of color: A debate on race in the U. S. workplace. Harvard Business Review 75 118-132. Tuppen, C. J. S. (1974). Dimensions of communicator credibility: An oblique solution. Speech Monoggphs, 41, (August), 253-260. West, C. (1993). Race Matters. Boston: Beacon Press. Wu, C., & Shaffer, D. R. (1987). Susceptibility to persuasive appeals as a function of source credibility and prior experience with the attitude object. Journal of Personalig and Social Psychology, 52, (4), 677-688. 41 | l l ill lll‘ ll “lll‘llll l 1293 02563 610