. v 13F: 9. h. t. 5.1 r I . .Jn..am«.j.z. -. yflpaflufl... LR uttct . . . $59,...A, . z u .4 . K 3»: I... .. I .. .7 3:31.. ‘ i . . RI}?! 0 - ‘ . . .u‘ T. .. , . ‘ . ,v. THESIS .10 0‘ LIBRARIES MICHIGAN STATE UNIVERS ITY EAST LANSING, MICH 48824-1048 This is to certify that the dissertation entitled EFFECTS OF DIFFERENTIATION ON COLLEGE STUDENT DRINKING presented by CHRISTOPHER R. LATTY has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for the Doctoral degree in Family and Child Ecolgg! MTWF/Ld. Major Professor‘s Signatfire ’7'22— 05’ Date MSU Is an Affirmative Action/Equal Opportunity Institution “— PLACE IN REI'URN BOX to remove this checkout from your record. To AVOID FINES return on or before date due. MAY BE RECALLED with earlier due date if requested. DATE DUE DATE DUE DATE DUE AUG 0 6 2007 APR 0 2 290; :5Z409 2/05 mlClFlCIDeteDuehdd-pds EFFECTS OF DIFFERENTIATION 0N COLLEGE STUDENT DRINKING By Christopher R. Latty A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Family and Child Ecology 2005 ABSTRACT EFFECTS OF DIFFERENTIATION ON COLLEGE STUDENT DRINKING By Christopher R. Latty This study examined if Bowen’s theory of differentiation was related to general alcohol use (GAU) and binge drinking among college students. It was hypothesized that students with lower levels of differentiation would be more likely to have greater amounts of general alcohol consumption and a higher prevalence of binge drinking. It was also predicted that students with lower levels of differentiation would have greater amounts of general alcohol use and binge drinking to the extent that they had greater: perceptions of other Michigan State University (MSU) student’s alcohol use, perceptions of their best friend at MSU’s use, expectations of alcohol use as a tension reducer and social lubricant, and were a child of an alcoholic (COA). A total of 447 participants (246 females, 201 males) between the ages of 18 and 22 were analyzed for this study. Due to a high level of skewness in the continuous variables, GAU and binge drinking were each transformed into four categories. The GAU categories were Abstainers, Low Drinkers, Moderate Drinkers, and Higher Drinkers; the binge drinking categories were Abstainers, Nonbinging Drinkers, Occasional Binge Drinkers, and Frequent Binge Drinkers. Overall, the hypotheses received little support from the logistic regression analyses. The hypothesis of differentiation and GAU was only supported in the Drinkers versus Abstainers comparison for males. Following the hypothesized relationship, as levels of differentiation increased in males the likelihood they drank alcohol decreased. The differentiation hypothesis was not supported by any of the binge drinking comparisons. Again, the majority of the moderator hypotheses were not supported by the model. Additionally, when significant interactions were found it was interpreted that differentiation acted as a moderating variable. The only significant interaction in the GAU analyses was with differentiation and perception of MSU student alcohol use. This interaction was significant for the High versus Moderate Drinker comparison (males) and the High versus Low Drinker comparison (females). In relation to binge drinking the only significant interactions were for the female analyses. There was a significant interaction between perceptions of MSU student use with Frequent Binge Drinkers versus Nonbinging Drinkers. For these interactions it was interpreted that higher levels of dilferentiation served as a protective factor against the disparate risk factors analyzed, as students with lower levels of differentiation were more vulnerable to being in the higher drinking category in relation to the risk factor. The other significant interaction was with COA status in the Frequent versus Occasional Binge Drinking comparison. Paradoxically, high levels of differentiation appeared to be a risk factor with COAs, as females were nearly two-and-a-half times more likely to be Frequent versus Occasional Binge Drinkers when they had higher levels of differentiation. Explanations for the respective interactions, clinical implications and recommendations for future research are provided. COFyright by Christopher R. Latty 2005 This work is dedicated to Sarah and Olivia. Your patience, love, and support allowed me to complete a long and wonderful journey. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS I am deeply indebted to a number of individuals that allowed me to achieve this degree. My wife, Sarah, has shown an invaluable level of patience, support, and compassion throughout my academic career. You are an incredible person and I thank you for all that you did to help me achieve this goal. I want to thank my daughter for bringing such joyous light into a journey that was marked by many dark moments. My parents, Dale and Pat, have provided a lifetime of support and unconditional love that afforded me an opportunity to explore a road of academic curiosities which led me this degree. I would also like to thank my brother, Shawn, and fi'iends Matt, Whitney, Jim, Scott, Jessica, and Henry for providing respite and much needed fun and balance throughout my graduate training. I also need to thank my colleagues and mentors Kathleen, Lori, Christie, Tianna, Al, Jason, Brandon, Dr. Lee, Dr. Soderman, and Dr. Bristor who supported, challenged, and inspired me to be a better teacher, researcher, and clinician. I am also indebted to NIAAA and the Department of Family and Child Ecology whose fimding was an invaluable resource that allowed me to invest the time and energy needed to complete this dissertation. Finally, I need to thank my committee for all of their time, patience, and energy. Dr. Meece and Dr. Luster provided many insights along the way regarding data analysis and editing. Dr. Fitzgerald, my dissertation co-chair, supported, challenged, and inspired me to stretch and develop my skills as a researcher. Your sponsorship of my NIAAA fellowship and mentoring were a fantastic capstone to my graduate training. I cannot thank you enough for all that you did for me. Finally, I would like to extend my thanks to Dr. Carolan, my major professor, dissertation co-chair, and wearer of so many hats in my academic development. You were an unbelievably supportive constant throughout a time of much transition. I will always cherish your encouragement, support, guidance, and sense of humor. Thank you. vii TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES ......................................................................................................... xi LIST OF FIGURES ....................................................................................................... xvii CHAPTER ONE Introduction .................................................................................................................... 1 Statement of the Problem ............................................................................................... 3 General Alcohol Use ................................................................................................ 3 Binge Drinking ......................................................................................................... 5 Importance of the Problem ............................................................................................. 5 Theoretical Framework ..... 5 Human Ecological Theory ....................................................................................... 5 Bowen Theory .......................................................................................................... 7 Conceptual Models .................................................................................................. 12 General Alcohol Use Model .............................................................................. 14 Binge Drinking Model ....................................................................................... 15 Research Questions .................................................................................................. 16 General Alcohol Use .......................................................................................... 16 Binge Drinking ................................................................................................... 16 CHAPTER TWO Introduction .................................................................................................................... 17 Literature Review ........................................................................................................... 17 College Student Drinking ........................................... . ............................................. 17 Binge Drinking ......................................................................................................... 18 Ecological Influences on College Student Drinking ................................................ 22 Individual Factors .............................................................................................. 22 Space and Location Influences .......................................................................... 23 Family of Origin Influences ............................................................................... 23 Family of Origin Influences through a Bowenian Lens ..................................... 25 Role of Anxiety and Stress on Drinking ............................................................ 27 Perceptions of Others’ Alcohol Use .................................................................. 32 Summary .................................................................................................................. 34 CHAPTER THREE Introduction .................................................................................................................... 35 Methods .......................................................................................................................... 35 Conceptual and Operational Definitions .................................................................. 35 Research Objectives ................................................................................................. 39 Hypotheses ............................................................................................................... 41 Research Design ....................................................................................................... 43 Measures ........................................................................................................................ 44 viii Differentiation .......................................................................................................... 44 Parental History of Alcohol Use. ............................................................................. 45 General Alcohol Use ................................................................................................ 46 Binge Drinking ......................................................................................................... 46 Expectancies of Alcohol as a Tension Reducer and Social Lubricant ..................... 47 Perception of the Average Michigan State Student’s Alcohol Use ......................... 47 Perception of Their Best Friend’s Alcohol Use ....................................................... 47 Demographic Information ........................................................................................ 48 Sample ............................................................................................................................ 48 Data Analysis ................................................................................................................. 50 CHAPTER FOUR Introduction .................................................................................................................... 53 Analyses of General Alcohol Use .................................................................................. 53 Differentiation and GAU ......................................................................................... 54 Four-F actors of the DSI-R and GAU ....................................................................... 56 Moderator Analyses ................................................................................................. 57 Comprehensive Models ........................................................................................... 61 Analyses of Binge Drinking ........................................................................................... 67 Differentiation and Binge Drinking ......................................................................... 68 Four-Factors of the DSI—R and Binge Drinking ...................................................... 68 Moderator Analyses ................................................................................................. 70 Comprehensive Models ........................................................................................... 72 CHAPTER FIVE Introduction .................................................................................................................... 83 Discussion of Results ..................................................................................................... 83 Differentiation and Alcohol Consumption. .............................................................. 84 F our-F actors of the DSI-R and Alcohol Consumption ............................................ 85 Moderator Hypotheses ............................................................................................. 87 Limitations ..................................................................................................................... 93 Recommendations for Future Research ......................................................................... 94 Clinical Implications ...................................................................................................... 97 Summary ........................................................................................................................ 98 REF EEN CES ................................................................................................................. 100 APPENDICES Appendix A: Differentiation of Self Inventory — Revised (DSI-R) .............................. ll 1 Appendix B: Analysis of the DSI-R with College Students ......................................... 116 Appendix C: Children of Alcoholics Screening Test (CAST) ...................................... 129 Appendix D: Children of Alcoholics Screening Test—Mother (M-CAST) ................... 131 Appendix E: Children of Alcoholics Screening Test-Father (F-CAST) ..................... 134 Appendix F: Demographic Information and Alcohol Use ........................................... 136 Appendix G: Expectancies of Alcohol Use .................................................................. 140 Appendix H: Consent Form .......................................................................................... 144 Appendix I: Debriefing Form ...................................................................................... 145 Appendix J: Transformation of Continuous Alcohol Measures into Categorical Data ............................................................................ 147 Appendix K: Individual Variable Descriptions ............................................................. 154 Appendix L: Analyses of General Alcohol Use ........................................................... 165 Appendix M: Analyses of Binge Drinking .................................................................... 220 Table 1. Table 2. Table 3. Table 4. Table 5. Table 6. Table 7. Table 8. Table 9. Table 10. Table 11. Table 12. Table 13. Table 14. Table 15. Table 16. Table 17. Table 18. LIST OF TABLES Logistic Regression Predicting Male Students that High versus Moderate Drinkers - Perceptions of MSU Student Use .............................. 58 Logistic Regression Predicting Female Students that High versus Low Drinkers - Perceptions of MSU Student Use .............................................. 59 Comprehensive Models for Male GAU Categories ..................................... 64 Comprehensive Models for Female GAU Categories ................................. 65 Logistic Regression Predicting Male Students thatare Frequent Bingers versus Occasional Bingers - Four Factors ................................................... 69 Logistic Regression Predicting Female Students that are Frequent Bingers versus Nonbingers - Perceptions of MSU Student Use ................. 71 Logistic Regression Predicting Female Students that Are Frequent Bingers versus Occasional Bingers - COA Status ...................................... 72 Comprehensive Models for Male Binge Drinking Categories .................... 78 Comprehensive Models for Female Binge Drinking Categories ................. 79 Summary of the Significant Individual Predictors Specifically Related to Differentiation for GAU .......................................................................... 82 Means and Distribution for the Total DSI-R and Subscales ........................ 117 Correlations for the Total DSI-R and Subscales .......................................... 1 l7 Correlation Matrix for the DSI-R Item Clusters .......................................... 121 Means and Distribution for the Revised Total DSI-R and Subscales .......... 123 Correlations for the Revised Total DSI-R and Subscales ............................ 123 Correlation Matrix'for the Revised DSI-R Item Clusters ............................ 124 Frequency of Binge Drinking Categories by Gender .................................. 152 Frequency of GAU Categories by Gender ................................................... 153 Table 19. Table 20. Table 21. Table 22. Table 23. Table 24. Table 25. Table 26. Table 27. Table 28. Table 29. Table 30. Table 31. Table 32. Table 33. Table 34. Table 35. Table 36. Table 37. Table 38. Frequency of Race by Gender ..................................................................... 155 Frequency of Parental Alcoholism by Gender ............................................. 156 Frequency of Parental Alcoholism and Binge Drinking by Gender ............ 157 Frequency of Parental Alcoholism and GAU by Gender ............................ 157 Frequency of COA Status and Binge Drinking by Gender ......................... 158 Frequency of COA Status and GAU by Gender .......................................... 158 Frequency of Independent Variables by Gender ......................................... 160 Correlations for Independent Variables - Male Subjects ............................ 162 Correlations for Independent Variables — Female Subjects ......................... 163 VIP for Independent Variables .................................................................... 164 Logistic Regression Predicting Students that Drink - Differentiation ........ 167 Logistic Regression Predicting Students that Drink - Four Factors ............ 167 Logistic Regression Predicting Students that Drink — Perceptions of MSU Student Use ........................................................................................ 169 Logistic Regression Predicting Students that Drink — Perceptions of Best Friend’s Use ......................................................................................... 172 Logistic Regression Predicting Students that Drink - COA Status ............. 174 Logistic Regression Predicting Students that Drink — Expectations of Tension Reduction ....................................................................................... 176 Logistic Regression Predicting Students that Drink - Expectations of Social Lubrication ........................................................................................ 179 Logistic Regression Predicting Students that Drink — Comprehensive Model ........................................................................................................... 181 Logistic Regression Predicting Students that are Moderate versus Low Drinkers — Differentiation ............................................................................ 182 Logistic Regression Predicting Students that are Moderate versus Low Drinkers—Four Factors ............................................................................... 182 xii Table 39. Table 40. Table 41. Table 42. Table 43. Table 44. Table 45. Table 46. Table 47. Table 48. Table 49. Table 50. Table 51. Table 52. Table 53. Logistic Regression Predicting Students that are Moderate versus Low Drinkers - Perceptions of MSU Student Use .............................................. 184 Logistic Regression Predicting Students that are Moderate versus Low Drinkers — Perceptions of Best Friend’s Use ............................................... 186 Logistic Regression Predicting Students that are Moderate versus Low Drinkers — COA Status ................................................................................ 187 Logistic Regression Predicting Students that are Moderate versus Low Drinkers - Expectations of Tension Reduction ........................................... 189 Logistic Regression Predicting Students that are Moderate versus Low Drinkers- Expectations of Social Lubrication ............................................ 193 Logistic Regression Predicting Students that are Moderate versus Low Drinkers - Comprehensive Model ............................................................... 194 Logistic Regression Predicting Students that are High versus Low Drinkers — Differentiation ............................................................................ 195 Logistic Regression Predicting Students that High versus Low Drinkers - Four Factors .............................................................................................. 195 Logistic Regression Predicting Students that High versus Low Drinkers - Perceptions of MSU Student Use ............................................................. 197 Logistic Regression Predicting Students that High versus Low Drinkers - Perceptions of Best Friend’s Use .............................................................. 199 Logistic Regression Predicting Students that are High versus Low Drinkers — COA Status ................................................................................ 201 Logistic Regression Predicting Students that High versus Low Drinkers - Expectations of Tension Reduction ........................................... 203 Logistic Regression Predicting Students that High versus Low Drinkers - Expectations of Social Lubrication ........................................................... 205 Logistic Regression Predicting Students that High versus Low Drinkers - Comprehensive Model .............................................................................. 207 Logistic Regression Predicting Students that High versus Moderate Drinkers — Differentiation ............................................................................ 209 xiii Table 54. Table 55. Table 56. Table 57. Table 58. Table 59. Table 60. Table 61. Table 62. Table 63. Table 64. Table 65. Table 66. Table 67. Table 68. Logistic Regression Predicting Students that High versus Moderate Drinkers — Four Factors ............................................................................... 209 Logistic Regression Predicting Students that High versus Moderate Drinkers - Perceptions of MSU Student Use .............................................. 212 Logistic Regression Predicting Students that are High versus Moderate Drinkers — Perceptions of Best Friend’s Use ............................................... 214 Logistic Regression Predicting Students that are High versus Moderate Drinkers - COA Status ................................................................................ 215 Logistic Regression Predicting Students that are High versus Moderate Drinkers - Expectations of Tension Reduction ........................................... 216 Logistic Regression Predicting Students that are High versus Moderate Drinkers - Expectations of Social Lubrication ............................................ 218 Logistic Regression Predicting Students that are High versus Moderate Drinkers — Comprehensive Model ............................................................... 219 Logistic Regression Predicting Students that are Occasional Bingers versus Nonbingers — Differentiation ............................................................ 222 Logistic Regression Predicting Students that are Bingers versus Nonbingers — Four Factors ........................................................................... 222 Logistic Regression Predicting Students that are Bingers versus Nonbingers - Perceptions of MSU Student Use .......................................... 223 Logistic Regression Predicting Students that are Bingers versus Nonbingers — Perceptions of Best Friend’s Use .......................................... 225 Logistic Regression Predicting Students that are Bingers versus Nonbingers -— COA Status ............................................................................ 227 Logistic Regression Predicting Students that are Bingers versus Nonbingers - Expectations of Tension Reduction ....................................... 229 Logistic Regression Predicting Students that are Bingers versus Nonbingers - Expectations of Social Lubrication ....................................... 233 Logistic Regression Predicting Students that are Bingers versus Nonbingers - Comprehensive Model .......................................................... 234 xiv Table 69. Table 70. Table 71. Table 72. Table 73. Table 74. Table 75. Table 76. Table 77. Table 78. Table 79. Table 80. Table 81. Table 82. Table 83. Logistic Regression Predicting Students that are Occasional Bingers versus Nonbingers — Differentiation ............................................................ 236 Logistic Regression Predicting Students that are Occasional Bingers versus Nonbingers — Four Factors ............................................................... 236 Logistic Regression Predicting Students that are Occasional Bingers versus Nonbingers - Perceptions of MSU Student Use ............................... 237 Logistic Regression Predicting Students that are Occasional Bingers versus Nonbingers — Perceptions of Best Friend’s Use ............................... 238 Logistic Regression Predicting Students that are Occasional Bingers versus Nonbingers - COA Status ................................................................ 239 Logistic Regression Predicting Students that are Occasional Bingers versus Nonbingers - Expectations of Tension Reduction ........................... 242 Logistic Regression Predicting Students that are Occasional Bingers versus Nonbingers — Expectations of Social Lubrication ............................ 243 Logistic Regression Predicting Students that are Occasional Bingers versus Nonbingers - Comprehensive Model ............................................... 245 Logistic Regression Predicting Students that are Frequent Bingers versus Nonbingers - Differentiation ............................................................ 246 Logistic Regression Predicting Students that are Frequent Bingers versus Nonbingers — Four Factors ............................................................... 246 Logistic Regression Predicting Students that are Frequent Bingers versus Nonbingers - Perceptions of MSU Student Use ............................... 248 Logistic Regression Predicting Students that are Frequent Bingers versus Nonbingers - Perceptions of Best Friend’s Use ............................... 251 Logistic Regression Predicting Students that are Frequent Bingers versus Nonbingers - COA Status ................................................................ 252 Logistic Regression Predicting Students that are Frequent Bingers versus Nonbingers — Expectations of Tension Reduction ........................... 255 Logistic Regression Predicting Students that are Frequent Bingers versus Nonbingers — Expectations of Social Lubrication ............................ 258 XV Table 84. Table 85. Table 86. Table 87. Table 88. Table 89. Table 90. Table 91. Table 92. Logistic Regression Predicting Students that are Frequent Bingers versus Nonbingers — Comprehensive Model ............................................... 259 Logistic Regression Predicting Students that are Frequent Bingers versus Occasional Bingers — Differentiation ............................................... 261 Logistic Regression Predicting Students that are Frequent Bingers versus Occasional Bingers - Four Factors ................................................... 261 Logistic Regression Predicting Students that are Frequent Bingers versus Occasional Bingers — Perceptions of MSU Student Use .................. 263 Logistic Regression Predicting Students that Are Frequent Bingers versus Occasional Bingers — Perceptions of Best Friend’s Use .................. 265 Logistic Regression Predicting Students that Are Frequent Bingers versus Occasional Bingers - COA Status .................................................... 268 Logistic Regression Predicting Students that Are Frequent Bingers versus Occasional Bingers - Expectations of Tension Reduction ............... 269 Logistic Regression Predicting Students that Are Frequent Bingers versus Occasional Bingers - Expectations of Social Lubrication ............... 271 Logistic Regression Predicting Students that are Frequent Bingers versus Occasional Bingers - Comprehensive Model ................................... 273 xvi Figure 1. Figure 2. Figure 3. Figure 4. Figure 5. Figure 6. Figure 7. Figure 8. Figure 9. Figure 10. Figure 11. Figure 12. Figure 13. Figure 14. Figure 15. LIST OF FIGURES Conceptual Model for College Students’ General Alcohol Use ................ 14 Conceptual Model for College Students’ Binge Drinking ......................... 15 The differentiation and perception of MSU student alcohol use interaction for the comparison of male High versus Moderate Drinkers... 60 The differentiation and perception of MSU student alcohol use interaction for female High versus Low Drinkers ...................................... 62 The differentiation and perception of best Friend at MSU’s alcohol use interaction for female High versus Low Drinkers ...................................... 66 The differentiation and perception of MSU student alcohol use interaction for female Frequent Binge Drinkers versus Nonbinging Drinkers ...................................................................................................... 73 The differentiation and COA status interaction for female Frequent versus Occasional Binge Drinkers ............................................................. 74 The differentiation and expectations of tension reduction interaction for male Frequent versus Occasional Binge Drinkers ..................................... 77 The differentiation and expectations of tension reduction interaction for female Binge Drinkers versus Nonbinging Drinkers ................................. 80 The differentiation and expectations of tension reduction interaction for female Occasional Binge Drinkers versus Nonbinging Drinkers .............. 81 Four-F actor Analysis of the DSI-R with Males and Females .................... 125 Differentiation as a Higher Order Latent Factor with Males and Females ....................................................................................................... 126 F our-F actor Analysis of the Revised DSI-R with Males and Females ...... 127 Differentiation as a Higher Order Latent Factor with Males and Females in the Revised DSI-R ................................................................... 128 General Alcohol Use (Quantity X Frequency) of Male Drinkers for the Past 30 Days .............................................................................................. 148 Figure 16. General Alcohol Use (Quantity X Frequency) of Female Drinkers for the Past 30 Days ......................................................................................... 149 Figure 17. Frequency of Male Binge Drinking Over the Past 2 Weeks ...................... 150 Figure 18. Frequency of Female Binge Drinking Over the Past 2 Weeks ................... 151 xviii Chapter One Introduction Alcohol consumption has a high rate of prevalence among college students. Estimates indicate that around 81 percent of college students will drink alcohol during the current year, and approximately 66 percent of these students will have consumed alcohol in the past month (Johnston, O'Malley, Bachman, & Schulenberg, 2004). Additionally, many of these students will consume alcohol to the point of intoxication. According to Woltcrsdorf (1997), anywhere from 26 to 48% of college students drink to intoxication during any given month. Moreover, he asserts that nearly 4% of college students drink daily. Furthermore, it is estimated that within the past 2 weeks over 40% of college students will have engaged in binge drinking (Wechsler, Lee, Kuo, et al., 2002). Estimates indicate around 31% of college students have a DSM-IV diagnosis of abuse, and 6% have a diagnosis of dependence (Knight et al., 2002). This high prevalence of abuse puts students at greater risk for academic problems, sexual abuse, risky sexual behaviors, physical injury, assault, violence, and death. As college students are in a period of developmental transition into young adulthood, it is important to understand the continued relationship college students have with their families of origin. This study sought to understand how the relationship with one’s parents affects the decision-makingfprocesses of alcohol consumption. Central constructs to this study were Bowen’s (1966) concepts of differentiation and triangling. Differentiation exists on inter- and intrapersonal levels. Intrapersonally, differentiation refers to a person’s ability to remain clear-minded and objective in the face of anxiety rather tlmn becoming emotionally reactive. It is also an interpersonal dynamic reflecting an individual’s ability to maintain a sense-of-self while remaining emotionally connected in relationships. Individuals with low levels of differentiation struggle to form identities with convictions and are more likely to adopt the beliefs and values of those for whose approval and closeness they yearn. Moreover, they are dependent on others for validation and ofien lack a firm foundation of self belief and approval. Therefore, when there is some form of relational anxiety (e.g., a criticism) individuals with lower levels of differentiation are not able to tolerate this stress and become emotionally reactive. When emotional disequilibrium occurs in a dyad, poorly differentiated individuals are likely to triangle in other people or things to diffuse this anxiety. For example, a couple on the cusp of fighting about themselves may channel this energy into a conflict about their children. Moreover, individuals may triangle things such as work or alcohol as a means to difl‘use the relational tension building in the relationship, resulting in a couple having conflict over the object rather than over their relationship. Patterns of differentiation are acquired in families and influence how anxiety is tolerated and whether triangulation will serve as a dysfunctional means to alleviate the anxiety. Accordingly, the more differentiated individuals are, the more likely they are to tolerate anxiety. Greater levels of differentiation result in greater potential to objectively respond to anxiety, decreasing the likeliness that an individual will need to triangle someone or something to regulate anxiety. The central hjpothesis of this study is that there is a relationship between dtflcrentiation and alcohol consumption in college students. It is crucial to understand the relationship between differentiation and alcohol consumption for this population for two reasons. First, college students experience a unique push-pull with their families of origin as they are likely geographically separated from their family for the first time. At this time, college students are at a developmental point of transition from adolescence to young adulthood and may not be able to tolerate the relational anxiety with their parents while at school. Secondly, college students may not be able to tolerate the anxiety of interpersonal relationships with peers and may triangle alcohol consumption, particularly via abuse and binge drinking, to manage their anxiety. As millions of students attend universities every year, the seriousness of this issue points to the necessity to understand ecological influences on their decisions to consume alcohol. This is especially true for those that acquire drinking as a means to cope with the new stressors of higher education. Attitudes and behavior towards alcohol, adopted while at college, may set a stage for a lifetime of dysfunctional usage and addiction. Statement of the Problem This study investigated the relationship between levels of differentiation and alcohol use for college students. Additionally, moderating effects on the relationship between differentiation and alcohol use were investigated for the following variables: status of being a child of an alcoholic (COA), perceptions of the average Michigan State University (MSU) student’s alcohol use, perceptions of best fiiend at MSU’s use, and the expectations of alcohol use as a tension reducer and social lubricant. General Alcohol Use Specifically, this model will investigate the following: 1. the relationship between differentiation, as measured by the Differentiation of Self Inventory—Revised (DSI-R, Skowron & Schmitt, 2003), and general alcohol consumption, as measured by quantity and frequency of alcohol use over the past 30 days . the relationship between the four factors of the DSI—R (Emotional Reactivity, 1 Position, Emotional Cutoff, and Fusion with Others) with general alcohol use . the moderating effects of COA status, as measured by parent-specific versions of the Children of Alcoholics Screening Test (Jones, 1983), on the relationship between differentiation and general alcohol use . the moderating effects of perceptions of the average MSU student’s alcohol use, as measured by students’ perceptions of the quantity and frequency of alcohol use over the past 30 days for the average MSU student, on the relationship between differentiation and general alcohol use . the moderating effects of perceptions of their best friend at MSU’s alcohol use, as measmed by students’ perceptions of the quantity and frequency of alcohol use over the past 30 days for their best fiiend at MSU, on the relationship between differentiation and general alcohol consumption . the moderating effects of expectations of alcohol serving as a tension reducer, as measured by the Tension reduction subscale (Kushner, Sher, Wood, & Wood, 1994), on the relationship between differentiation and general alcohol consumption . the moderating effects of expectations of alcohol serving as a social lubricant. as measured by the Social Lubrication subscale (Kushner et al., 1994), on the relationship between differentiation and general alcohol consumption Binge Drinking The outcome of binge drinking will also be investigated using the same seven points of investigation as mentioned for general alcohol use. Importance of the Problem This project is innovative in that it incorporates the systemic concept of differentiation with one’s family of origin to individual decision-making processes, which has not been previously applied to the understanding of alcohol consumption. If the hypothesized relationship between levels of differentiation and alcohol consumption exists, not only will it prove to be a unique contribution to the alcohol consumption literature, but will also yield a greater understanding of how to further understand the decision-making processes of college students. Additionally, mental health professionals working with adolescents and adolescent substance use will gain empirical evidence for concepts comprising Bowen Theory. Theoretical Framework Human Ecological Theory Human ecological theory and a systemic perspective consider how an individual, a family, and their total environment are intimately intertwined with one another (Andrews, Bubolz, & Paolucci, 1930; Bristor, 1995; Bronfenbrenner, 1977, 1999; Bubolz, Eicher, & Sontag, 1979; Bubolz & Sontag, 1993; Griffore & Phenice, 2001; von Bertalanfl‘y, 1980). The family ecosystem is comprised of a collection of interdependent people interacting, sharing resources, goals, and space (Andrews et al., 1980; Bubolz & Sontag, 1993; Griffore & Phenice, 2001). As children within the family ecosystem mature, they are greatly influenced by familial subsystems (e.g., parents, siblings) and the respective rules, roles, boundaries, hierarchy, patterns of interaction, perceptions, and expectations of the greater family system (Andrews et al., 1980; Bristor, 1995; Bubolz & Sontag, 1993; Griffore & Phenice, 2001). Just as an individual’s development is interconnected with other members of her family ecosystem, they individual shares a connection with her surrounding environment and contexts (Andrews et al., 1980; Bronfenbrenner, 1977, 1999; Bubolz et al., 1979; Bubolz & Sontag, 1993; Lerner, 1991, 1992, 1993; Lerner & Galambos, 1998; Lerner & Lerner, 1987, 1989; von Bertalanffy, 1980). The development and wellbeing of individuals is greatly influenced by these unique contextual relationships. Although the field of alcohol research has become more systemically oriented, it only became commonplace within the past decade, both for the general population and specific to college students (Baer, 2002; Zucker, Fitzgerald, & Moses, 1995) (see Schulenberg & Maggs, 2002, for an ecological review of empirical studies for adolescent and young adults). This absence of ecological context is evident due to an emphasis on a minimal number of variables, absence of multivariate or interactive models, and a, lack of consideration of multiple developmental pathways to differing types of alcoholisms (Fitzgerald, Zucker, Puttler, Caplan, & Mun, 2000; Zucker, 1994; Zucker et al., 1995; Zucker, Reider, Ellis, & Fitzgerald, 1997). Consequently, while it is important to understand the role of individual variables on college students’ drinking, Zucker, Fitzgerald, and Moses (1995) suggest that risk for alcohol use be considered as a dynamic process consisting of varying degrees of risk for usage throughout the lifespan. Bowen Theory Around the time von Bertalanffy published his general systems theory in 1968, Murray Bowen was honing his systems theory of emotional systems in 1966 and 1967 (published as Anonymous in 1972). Bowen’s theory is grounded in four interconnected concepts: differentiation, triangles, emotional system, and the multigenerational transmission process. Drflerentiation. The driving force of Bowen’s theory is the concept of difl‘erentiation. As an intra-psychic construct, differentiation refers to the relationship between being able to utilize one’s cortex (thinking clearheadedly and objectively) over the limbic system (emotionally reactive) in the presence of anxiety. Accordingly, reactivity is inversely related to objectivity (Friedman, 1991). By maintaining a differentiated position, individuals can objectively think about stress and respond to it with a variety of clearheaded options, rather than merely being emotionally reactive. In the context of relationships, differentiation refers to how individuals and dyads maintain a sense of self while remaining connected to others. The ability to maintain a sense of self (basic-self) is described as having a firm notion of personal beliefs and values that will not be surrendered in the context of a relationship. Conversely, an undifferentiated individual makes decisions based on what feels like the right thing to do at the time rather than on a reasoned principle. The undifferentiated person has an . inability to form and develop his own convictions and will adopt those of significant others or popular ideologies. Unlike a differentiated person, who is able to generate unique “I believe” statements, an undifferentiated person will evidence laws or religion to define his dogma (Anonymous, 1972; Becvar & Becvar, 1993; Bowen, 1966). In the struggle between a desire for connectedness and separateness, an undifferentiated person fails to recognize where he ends and others begin, whereas a differentiated person is able to maintain independent thoughts and feelings within relationships (Wetchler & Piercy, 1996). At the crux of the struggle to remain differentiated is the ability to maintain a connection with others while at the same time being able to maintain individuality. Titelman (2003) describes this balance as “the ability to act for oneself without being selfish and the ability to act for others without being selfless” (p. 20). Bowen conceptualized differentiation as existing on a continuum with the outcome of complete differentiation as not realistically obtainable (Anonymous, 1972; Bowen, 1966). Individuals in the upper half of the continuum possess an increased ability to differentiate between objective reality and feelings and they primarily can filnction in ways that remain true to their sense of self—although some decisions are based on feelings in order to not risk disapproval fiom significant others. Finally, these individuals are much less reactive to praise and criticism as they have a much more firm identity, whereas people with lower levels of differentiation are highly dependent on others for external validation. Theoretically, people tend to couple with others who share similar levels of differentiation. When these levels are low, the two individuals become fused with one another and little diflerentiation of self remains for the individuals. Individuals with low levels of differentiation are dependent on others for approval and validation for a sense of self. Thus, they are more susceptible to have dysfunctional relationships and. symptoms to the extent that they experience disrupted emotional equilibrium and individual comfort levels through criticism, conflict, or relational anxiety from others (Bowen, 1966). Individuals are at greater risk for developing depressed, somatic, or alcohol related symptoms to the extent that they have lower levels of differentiation; conversely they can tolerate more intense anxiety as their differentiation levels increase (Friedman, 1991; Skowron & F riedlander, 1998). In a well differentiated relationship, each member of the couple “is more of an autonomous self: there is less emotional fusion in close relationships, less energy is needed to maintain self in the fusions, more energy is available for goal-directed activity, and more satisfaction is derived hour the directed activity” (Anonymous, 1972, p. 119). As anxiety or tension exceeds the comfort level of a relational dyad, it is more likely that another person or thing (e.g., alcohol or work) will be triangled into the relationship as a means of shifting the tension away from the dyad (Bowen, 1966; Friedman, 1991; McGoldrick & Carter, 2001; Titelman, 1998). Triangles. According to Bowen (1966, Anonymous, 1972), dyads within emotional systems are naturally unstable and will form a triangle, with another person (people) or object(s), when under an unmanageable amount of stress. The intensity of the triangle relationship is dependent on the level of differentiation as well as the importance of the relationship that is experiencing the anxiety (Anonymous, 1972). In the context of an extended family or a work environment, the system is comprised of multiple interlocking triangles. The push-and-pull forces of the desire for togetherness and individuality in a relationship create varying levels of stress and anxiety for dyads. As mentioned, when this anxiety or tension exceeds the comfort level of a relational dyad, it is more likely that a triangle will be brought into the relationship as a means of shifting the tension away from the dyad (Bowen, 1966; Friedman, 1991; McGoldrick & Carter, 2001; Titelman, 1998). Some of the ways in which a dyad could triangle in another person or object are: changing the conversation by talking about a third party, one or both individuals may bring another person in to take sides, one person continually venting her frustrations through a third person rather than directly with the other, one person shifting her energy to work or alcohol rather than toward the conflict with the other. No matter what the method of triangling is, the ultimate goal is to reduce the amount of tension, at the expense of dealing with it directly, as the ability to remain objective and nonreactive is lost. Although occasional triangling is not necessarily a dysfunctional behavior, it becomes problematic when it is utilized as a constant diversion that prevents individuals and couples from directly dealing with their problems. The emotional system This concept, originally termed undifferentiated ego mass (Bowen, 1966), refers to the overall level of differentiation, or fusion, in a system (e.g., the nuclear family emotional system) (Anonymous, 1972). As this concept is interrelated with difl‘erentiation, the extent of emotional fusion between spouses is a product of the level of difl‘erentiation within the individuals. Bowen (1966, Anonymous, 1972) identified three ways symptoms fi'om undifferentiated parental couples can be expressed in a nuclear family: marital conflict, projection to a child(ren), or dysflurction in a parent/spouse. Families vary greatly regarding which of the areas receive the dysfunction and in the degree to which it is shared across the areas. For example, if one member of the parental dyad merges his identity into the couple’s identity-losing his sense of self- then he would be likely to experience some form of dysfirnction (e.g., developing depression, somatic illness, or alcoholism), which may in turn prevent dysfimction from occurring in marital conflict or fiom being projected onto the children. 10 Multigenerational transmission The concepts of differentiation and triangles operate in concert within families through multigenerational transmission. As parents triangle children in order to diffuse anxiety within the marital relationship, children may be inducted into multigenerational transmission of firsed relationships. As children become more mature, they are consequently faced with the task of becoming more differentiated from their family in order to become higher functioning individuals (Titelman, 1998). Children who are fi'equently triangled at a young age into their parents’ relationship may develop lower levels of differentiation than their parents due to their stunted development as triangled individuals. If this child later marries, it will theoretically be to someone sharing a similar level of differentiation (Anonymous, 1972; Bowen, 1966) and he will have a child that may have an even lower level of differentiation. However, children are not automatically destined to be at lower levels of differentiation than their parents, other factors may disrupt this inevitably. In addition, greater levels of differentiation can be obtained via a lifelong process. Research with Bowen theory. Although this can be an important lens through - which to view symptom formation, few studies have explored this postulated relationship in college students by examining the degree to which differentiation influences disparate types of psychopathology, including alcohol use. Protinsky and Gilkey (1996) found a relationship between females’ sense of individuation and self-esteem, personal health, and grade point average. Bartle-Haring, Rosen, and Stith (2002) found that possessing lower levels of differentiation with one’s mother was related to increased psychological symptoms and increased reports of stressful life events. Research has also found consequences of triangulation within families to be related to intimacy and academic ll difficulties in young adults, and substance abuse in adolescents (Larson & Wilson, 1998). Studies evidence that individuals with higher levels of differentiation have been found to be more flexible, better able to cope with stress, and have less chronic anxiety (Larson & Wilson, 1998; Skowron & F riedlander, 1998). Additionally, Elieson and Rubin (2001) found that college students with depression had lower levels of differentiation than did traditional student groups. These reports indicate a need to further examine how individuation in college students influences personal health, particularly how well-being is compromised through decisions to triangle alcohol as a means to cope with anxiety. Conceptual Models The model for this study is grounded in the concepts of Bowen theory via two essential constructs relevant to this study—differentiation and triangulationDifferentiation influences how an individual tolerates anxiety, whereas triangling is a dysfunctional means to alleviate the anxiety. According to this theory, the more differentiated individuals are, the more likely they are to tolerate anxiety, resulting in greater potential for an objective response to anxiety. More differentiated individuals will be less likely to exhibit a need to triangle someone or something to regulate anxiety. Therefore, the general model of this study is that individuals with higher levels of differentiation will be less likely to triangle alcohol as a means to diffuse their anxiety. The relationship between differentiation and alcohol use is also expected to be moderated by child of an alcoholic (COA) status, perceptions of the average MSU student’s alcohol consumption, and the perception of best friend at MSU’s alcohol consumption, expectations of alcohol as a tension reducer, and expectations of alcohol as a social lubricant. 12 It is expected that children of alcoholics will be more likely to triangle alcohol as they will possess a history of having parents model alcohol use as a triangle to tolerate anxiety. The moderating effect of being a COA is further based upon the premise of multigenerational transmission. As Bowen theory suggests, children have similar levels of differentiation as their parents; it is assumed that individuals with low levels of differentiation have parents who also have low levels of differentiation. It is expected that alcoholic parents with low levels of differentiation chose alcohol as a triangle, therefore providing a model of managing stress-COA students who are away from home may feel comfortable triangling alcohol in relation to parent modeling and due to alcohol’s role as an integral part of the college culture. It is expected that less-differentiated students will generally consume greater amounts of alcohol to the extent that they perceive greater amounts of student use presenting as normative means by which to diffuse anxiety. Finally, it is expected that students possessing lower levels of differentiation will have greater amounts of general alcohol consumption to the extent that they have higher expectations that alcohol will serve as a social lubricant or a tension reducer as a means to help tolerate relational anxiety (see Figure 1, p. 14, and Figure 2, p. 15). I3 Parental Alcohol Use L Level of I genial] Differentiation I co 0. Consumptlon Perceptions of the Average MSU Student’s Use Level of L 216mm Differentiation a co 0. Consumptlon Perceptions of their Best Friend at MSU’s Use Level of I 23mg Differentiation T on 0. Consumptlon Expectations of Alcohol as a Tension Reducer Level of J filmefi Differentiation ' co 0 . Consumptlon Expectations of Alcohol as a Social Lubricant Level of l finial Differentiation ' co 0. Consumptlon Figure 1. Conceptual model for college students’ general alcohol use. 14 Parental Alcohol Use l _ Level of Binge Differentiation Drinking Perceptions of the Average MSU Student’s Use Level of Binge Differentiation 4 a Drinking Perceptions of their Best Friend at MSU’s Use Level of I _ Binge Differentiation ' Dn'nking Expectations of Alcohol as a Tension Reducer Level of L Binge Differentiation 4 Drinking Expectations of Alcohol as a Social Lubricant Level Of 1 Binge Differentiation Drinking Figure 2. Conceptual model for college students’ binge drinking 15 Research Questions The overarching research questions for this study are based on two models. The review of literature in Chapter Two will support and guide the relationships for the models to be tested. General Alcohol Use for College Students Is there a significant relationship between differentiation and general alcohol consumption for college students? Are there moderating effects on the relationship between differentiation and general alcohol consumption for the following variables: COA status, perceptions of the average MSU student’s alcohol consumption, perceptions of best friend at MSU’s alcohol consumption, expectations that alcohol will serve as a tension reducer or a social lubricant? Binge Drinking for College Students Is there a significant relationship between differentiation and binge drinking for college students? Are there moderating effects on the relationship between differentiation and binge drinking for the following variables: COA status, perceptions of the average MSU student’s alcohol consumption, perceptions of best fiiend at MSU’s alcohol consumption, expectations that alcohol will serve as a tension reducer or a social lubricant? 16 Chapter Two Chapter Two presents a review of literature regarding general trends for college student alcohol consumption and provides a description of the concept and consequences of binge drinking. The role of individual and ecological factors will be considered along with the role of anxiety and stress. Additionally, evidence will be provided for the inclusion of the moderating variables of COA status, perceptions of others’ alcohol use, and expectations of alcohol use for this study’s models. Literature Review College Student Drinking Adolescence is a time when many individuals are at risk for developing problematic drinking behaviors. This is particularly true for adolescents who are attending college. Not only are many of these students away from parental supervision for the first time, but also there is an increased availability of alcohol to be found at college compared to high school, even for those who are legally underage (Dreer, Ronan, Ronan, Dush, & Elliott, 2004; Wechsler, Lee, Nelson, & Kuo, 2002; Zucker et al., 1997). At most colleges and universities, there is a normative component in the student culture involving alcohol consumption for reasons to socialize, relax, celebrate, and for pleasure (Demers et al., 2002; Dreer et al., 2004; Schulenberg & Maggs, 2002; Wechsler, Davenport, Dowdall, Moeykens, & Castillo, 1994; Wechsler, Isaac, Grodstein, & Sellers, 1994; Zucker et al., 1997). Many students will normatively drink alcohol and will never experience consequences of drinking beyond a hangover. Unfortunately, some students’ alcohol consumption may evolve into dangerous levels of consumption, result in various negative consequences, or begin a pattern for a life-long battle with dependence. 17 Beyond the potential long-term consequences of addiction or continued abuse, students are at risk for short-term effects of driving while intoxicated, experiencing a blackout, sufl‘ering an injury, poor academic performance, skipping class, dropping out, getting into fights, doing something they regretted, getting arrested, participating in unsafe sex, experiencing/initiating unwanted sexual activity, attempting/committing suicide, and unintentional death (Bishop, Lacour, Nutt, Yamada, & Lee, 2004; Cell, Shott, &- Morris, 1999; Kaplowitz & Campo, 2004; Larirner, Lydum, Anderson, & Turner, 1999; Presley, Meilman, & Lyerla, 1994; Smith, Wells, & Abdul-Salaam, 1997; Waite-O'Brien, 1992; Woltcrsdorf, 1997). Unfortunately, there appears to be a negative cycle between drinking and consequences as the more you drink the more likely you are to experience a consequence, and not knowing about the health risks of drinking is related to greater consumption (Jones, Harel, & Levinson, 1992). F urtherrnore, although everyone who drinks is susceptible to consequences, men are reported to experience them more oflen (Benton et al., 2004; Billingham, Wilson, & Gross, 1999). Binge Drinking Binge drinking has historically been defined as five or more drinks in a row at one sitting (Presley et al., 1994; Wechsler, Isaac, et al., 1994). However, after comparing odds ratios in the consequences of binge drinking with the traditional five drink standard, Wechsler, Dowdall, Davenport, and Rim (1995) assert a more equitable standard of four or more drinks be considered as binge drinking for females. This would account for differences in body mass and metabolism, and avoid the underreporting of binge drinking women. The standard for female binge drinking at four drinks in a row per setting has since been adopted in multiple studies (Benton et al., 2004; Wechsler, Davenport, et al., 1994; Wechsler, Dowdall, Davenport, & Castillo, 1995; Wechsler, Dowdall, Maenner, Gledhill-Hoyt, & Lee, 1998; Wechsler, Lee, Kuo, & Lee, 2000; Wechsler, Lee, Kuo, et al., 2002; Wechsler, Lee, Nelson, et al., 2002). Binge drinking is prevalent in nearly half of all college students. A series of national studies, covering 140 four-year colleges, have shown about 44% of college students binge drink-a stable percentage spanning 1993 (44%, Wechsler, Davenport, et al., 1994), 1997 (42.7%, Wechsler et al., 1998), 1999 (44%, Wechsler et al., 2000), and 2001 (44.4%, Wechsler, Lee, Kuo, et al., 2002). Although a similar rate of 42% was found in a national study by Presley (1994), this figure likely reflects an underestimated total of female binge drinkers as the study used the 5-drink standard for both genders. Although the overall percentage of college students that binge drink remains static over time, there are individual shifts in binging. Studying the changes of drinking behaviors during the transition of first- to second-year attendance at a Massachusetts college, Wechsler, Isaac, et al. (1994) found that 78% of males and 63% of females who consumed at binge levels during their first year continued to do so in their second year. Additionally, 37% of men and 19% of women who were nonbinge drinkers began to drink at binge levels dining their second year of attendance. The authors report that the overwhelming majority of the first-year binge drinkers were also binge drinkers in high school (62% of the females and 77% of the males). Although this may suggest a developmental trend for all adolescents, it appears to be specifically disturbing for college students. There is trend for college bound students to binge drink at lesser rates than non-college bound peers until they enter college, at which point they will binge drink at greater rates than their peers not attending college (Schulenberg et al., 2001). 19 This trend for increased prevalence of binge drinking with the transition into college was illustrated by Wechsler, Davenport, et al. (1994) as 10% of high school binge drinkers were no longer binging in college, while 22% of non-bingeing high school students started in college. Some of the documented consequences associated with binge drinking include: fighting, damaging property, experiencing blackouts, driving while intoxicated, being a passenger in a car with an intoxicated driver, doing something they regretted, missing class, acquiring a sexually transmitted infection, experiencing a sexual assault, having unplanned or unprotected sex, lower grade point average, and trouble with police (Benton et al., 2004; Clapp & McDonnell, 2000; Presley et al., 1994; Smith et al., 1997; Wechsler, Davenport, et al., 1994; Wechsler, Dowdall, Davenport, & Castillo, 1995; Wechsler et al., 1998; Wechsler et al., 2000; Wechsler, Lee, Kuo, et al., 2002). It is crucial to understand the prevalence of binge drinking among college students as Smith, Wells, et al. caution that there is a high risk of experiencing the consequences associated with binge drinking even if a person only has an isolated episode. Similar to alcohol consumption in general, infrequent binge drinking for college students has been described as a normative part of the university culture (Bladt, 2002). Nevertheless, some individuals may be at greater risk for this behavior than others. The 2001 estimates of Wechsler, Lee, Kuo, et al. (2002) indicate that the 40.9% of college females and 48.6% of college males binge drank at some level during the year and these gender differences do not appear to differ across races (O'Malley & Johnston, 2002). Underage students were reported to drink less frequently but were more likely to binge when they drank (57.8% of underage men to 41.9% of-age; 53% underage women to 20 37.2% of-age) (Wechsler, Lee, Nelson, et al., 2002). Additionally, regardless of age, students living in fi'aternity or sorority houses were more likely to binge drink than those living in any other type of residence (Wechsler et al., 1998; Wechsler, Lee, Nelson, et al., 2002). Caucasian students have been shown to be the most likely to binge drink, followed by Hispanic students, with African American students binging at much lower levels (O'Malley & Johnston, 2002). Frequent heavy binge drinkers have 19 times greater odds of meeting the DSM-IV criteria for dependence (Knight et al., 2002) than those who drink at lesser levels. Although it has never been studied specifically through a Bowenian lens, it can be inferred that people with lower levels of differentiation may be at greater risk for binging. Bladt (2002) examined mental health in first-semester college students that were classified as being abstainers, drinkers without binge behaviors, infrequent binge drinkers (l or 2 times in a 2-week period), and fiequent binge drinkers (3 or more times in a 2- week period). Frequently binging female students had significantly lower self-esteem than females in the other categories. Additionally, female frequent binge drinkers had a significantly greater feeling of insecure attachment than females who abstained fi'om drinking and male frequent binge drinkers. The author describes individuals with high levels of insecure attachment as having a strong and conflicted desire for closeness, consequently leaving them vulnerable to become enmeshed and concerned with abandonment. This study’s results are in concert with Bowen’s theory as individuals who are emotionally fused with significant others—being afiaid of criticism and abandonment, without being able to validate their own sense-of-self—would be unable to form secure attachments with others as they constantly react to others’ compliments and 21 criticisms. Consequently, these people would then be more likely to triangle someone or something, such as alcohol, as a means to tolerate inter- and intrapersonal anxiety. Ecological Influences on College Student Drinking Individual factors. Overall, college students appear to have a higher prevalence of drinking than their same age peers who are not attending college (O'Malley & Johnston, 2002). The most reported individual factor for the amount of alcohol consumed by college students is being male (Allgood-Merten, Lewinsohn, & Hops, 1990; Benton et al., 2004; Camatta & Nagoshi, 1995; Cooney & Nonnarnaker, 1992; Cotton, 1979; Demers et al., 2002; Dreer et al., 2004; Gisske & Adams, 1988; Grant et al., 1994; Harford, Wechsler, & Seibring, 2002; Johnson & Pandina, 2000; Jones et al., 1992; Lo, 1995; O'Malley & Johnston, 2002; Perkins & Berkowitz, 1986; Pullen, 1994; Sax, 1997; Sher, Walitzer, Wood, & Brent, 1991; Woltcrsdorf, 1997; Zucker et al., 1995). However, Demers et al. (2002) concluded that “the relationship between the frequency of drinking and the consumption per occasion is stronger for female than for male students” (p. 421). Moreover, recent changes in some methodologies have demonstrated that the gender gap may be smaller than typically reported when considering body weight and alcohol metabolism. Furthermore, as previously mentioned, after an analysis of the odds ratios of negative consequences with binge drinking, Wechsler, Dowdall, Davenport, and Rimm (1995) suggested changing the binge drinking criteria in women from 5 drinks (the same standard as men) to 4 drinks in a row per sitting for a more equitable standard. The change makes it more likely that binge drinking women will be accurately identified rather than be underreported in studies. Lange and Voas (2001) studied 18— to 33-year-old pedestrians returning from a night of drinking in Tijuana, Mexico. The authors found that _ 22 men and women had similar levels of mean blood alcohol concentration. Although females may be consuming less quantities of alcohol than males, they nevertheless may be drinking to similar levels of intoxication (Korcuska & Thombs, 2003) or possibly higher levels of intoxication and drinking problems (Fitzgerald, Zucker, Mun, Puttler, & Wong, 2002; Waite-O'Brien, 1992). Space and location influences. Additionally, the ecological consideration of location of dwelling place and where one drinks alcohol has an effect on consumption. College students who live away from their parents or are campus residents are more likely to consume alcohol (Cooney & Nonnamaker, 1992; Demers et al., 2002; Harford et al., 2002; Jones et al., 1992). Additionally, Harford et al. found that compared to women, men are more likely to attend dorm parties and off-campus parties and are less likely to attend off-campus bars. Interestingly, although a lower proportion of students attended fraternity/sorority parties than attended off-campus parties and bars, higher proportions of students attending fraternity/sorority parties chose to drink. Similarly, there is an incredible prevalence of binging for those who live in fratemity/sorority houses (83.4% in 1993 decreasing to 75.4% in 2001, see Wechsler, Lee, Kuo, et al., 2002). Family of origin influences. Fifteen percent of the nation’s children under 17 are estimated to live in a household with at least one adult with an abuse or dependence diagnosis over the past year (Grant, 2000; Zucker & Wong, in press). Studies indicate that approximately one-fifth (Rodney, 1995) to one-third (Landers & Hollingdale, 1988) of college students have at least one alcoholic parent. Although most children of alcoholics (COAs) will be resilient to the risks of being raised in an alcoholic family (O'Sullivan, 1992), many will experience a variety of 23 consequences. Ellis, Zucker, and Fitzgerald (1997) presented the importance of considering the roles of alcohol-specific and non-alcohol-specific risk factors on the lives of COAs. Research on non-alcohol-specific risk factors for COAs has revealed evidence of a long list of issues that include: learning disabilities, poorer intellectual fimctioning, conduct disorders, eating disorders, antisocial behavior, truancy, stress-related illnesses, anxiety, depression, increased rates of psychopathology, suicide attempts, inability to trust, denial of or inability to express feelings, an excessive sense of responsibility, poor communication, difliculty developing peer relationships, greater involvement in other drug use, experiencing sexual abuse, experiencing emotional neglect, experiencing family violence and conflict, having less cohesive families, greater levels of social isolation, having to undertake a greater number of parenting responsibilities, having greater parental marital instability, having parents with impaired cognitive abilities and comorbid psychopathology, and marrying someone who is an alcoholic (Black, Bucky, & Wilde- Padilla, 1986; Ellis et al., 1997; Garbarino & Strange, 1993; Hewes & Janikowski, 1998; Jones & Kinnick, 1995; Lawson, 1992; Marlatt ct al., 1998; O'Sullivan, 1992; Poon, Ellis, Fitzgerald, & Zucker, 2000; Rodney, 1995; Sher, Gershuny, Peterson, & Raskin, 1997; Yeatman, Bogart, Geer, & Sirridge, 1994; Zucker et al., 2000). A strong alcohol-specific consequence of being a COA also exists, as there is a relationship of COAs having drinking patterns closely related to their parents and greater alcohol usage than non-COAs (Ellis et al., 1997; Gisske & Adams, 1988; Gotham, Sher, & Wood, 2003; Jones & Kinnick, 1995; Yeatman et al., 1994), though this relationship was not supported by Engs (1990). This relationship also appears to transcend ethnicities as the transmission has also been illustrated in a sample of Afiican American students 24 (Rodney, 1995). Although the prevalence rates have some variability across studies, in a seminal review of 39 studies of COAs, Cotton (1979) found that alcoholics are more likely to have an alcoholic relative than nonalcoholics and that about 30% of any sample of alcoholics will have at least one alcoholic parent. A number of studies focus on the genetic heritability of alcoholism, however, some of the psychosocial variance related to multigenerational transmission of alcoholism in families has been described to be a learned behavior through modeling parental behaviors and adopting respective expectations of alcohol and its use (Ellis et al., 1997). Family of origin influences through a Bowenian lens. Reports demonstrate that COAs have less communication with their parents, have witnessed more family arguments (Sher et al., 1991), and have less social support and less healthy family environments (Rodney, 1995). From a Bowenian fiarnework, this lack of support and inability to tolerate anxiety and manage conflict results in a need to triangle an outside entity in order to diffuse interpersonal conflicts. Perhaps the triangled item of choice in many alcoholic families is alcohol itself, which then is modeled for future generations to observe as an appropriate coping mechanism. In the case of alcoholic families, the ability to maintain a difl‘erentiated sense-of-self would seem to be stymied by the typically inadequate responsive caregiving, retarded development of affect regulation, and poor self-esteem maintenance found in many COAs (Bladt, 2002). Furthermore, Clair and Genest (1987) found that COAs tended to use more emotion-focused, rather than problem-focused, strategies in coping with their problems. As poorly differentiated individuals lack the ability to maintain an objective presence in the face of anxiety, they 25 become more emotionally reactive and may have a need to triangle a person/object to alleviate their anxiety. COAs may be more apt to triangle alcohol as they having witnessed and adopted their parents’ coping strategies and attitudes of alcohol consumption in their own lives (Archambault, 1992; Howard, 1992). Ellis et al. (1997) surmise that increased conflict in alcoholic families may be the result of the combined poorer problem-solving skills and communication that are prevalent in so many of these families. This corresponds with Bowen’s theory that a lack of a difl‘erentiated position in families will result in poorer conflict management and an inability to remain objective in the face of anxiety or stress. Similarly, Garbarino and Strange (1993) found that adult COAs had less family expressiveness and high degrees of family conflict. The inability to maintain a differentiated sense of self is illustrated in their finding that COAs have difficulty expressing or identifying feelings. Karwacki and Bradley (1996) examined correlations between drinking motivation, coping responses, goal expectations, and family-of-ori gin drinking problems with college students' alcohol use. Their findings lend support to social learning theory and conclude that family models make significant contributions to excessive alcohol use in college students. Social learning theory is consistent with a Bowenian lens as children may observe their parents, with low levels of differentiation, triangle alcohol in order to shift the existing tension/anxiety away from the marital dyad. In turn, these children would then choose alcohol as a triangle to aid in their own anxiety tolerance. It is prudent to also consider how the concept of differentiation can be a useful lens through which to view familial influence on triangling alcohol for college students who may not necessarily be COAs. Haemmerlie, Steen, and Benedicto (1994) 26 investigated the role of conflictual independence (CI), adjustment to college, and consumption of alcohol by college students. The construct of conflictual independence was measm'ed fiem the Psychological Separation Inventory, which purports to delineate having a relationship with one’s parents that is free fi'om excessive guilt, anxiety, anger, and resentment-—all key Bowen concepts. A significant main effect was found for CI- Mother, as subjects with high drinking had lower CI-Mother scores than did subjects with low drinking scores. There were no significant findings for CI-Father or CI-Total. Additionally, when compared to how well students were adjusting to college, a significant positive relationship between Cl-Total was found with the overall scores of Student Adaptation to College Questionnaire. A significant correlation was also found between CI-Mother and CI-Father and the personal-emotional scores on the Student Adaptation to College Questionnaire. The authors concluded that lower C I fiom both parents was linked to the use of alcohol as a means to relieve emotional pressure. This relationship supports the concept of differentiation, as students with lower levels of conflictual independence were more likely to triangle alcohol as a way to tolerate their emotional anxiety. Role of anxiety and stress on drinking. As previously mentioned, the consumption of alcohol in the student population does not remain static throughout their academic career. Marlatt et al. (1998) report that expectancies for alcohol use may mediate peer and normative influences at this time. Expectancies of alcohol have been reported to have a mediating relationship in how COAs internalize parental drinking with their decisions about drinking (Reese, Chassin, & Molina, 1994). However, Ellis et a1. (1997) state that little is known about the formation of positive or negative expectancies with the severity 27 of parental alcoholism. Nonetheless, multiple studies have looked at the role of alcohol consumption related to anxiety. Essential to the framework of Bowen Theory is how an individual or dyad, acts in the face of anxiety. It is especially prudent to consider the role of anxiety tolerance for college students as Jones et al. (1992) state that perceived stress is greater for college students compared to their same age peers who are not enrolled in college. Bartlett (2002) reported on the adjustment of first year college students and showed 44.3% of students felt overwhelmed by the demands college had on their time (compared to 31.6% at the beginning of the year), 16% reported depressive symptoms (compared to 8.2%), and 44.9% rated their emotional health as being above average (compared to 52.4%). These reports indicate a trend for maladjustment for a large portion of students. Students with low levels of problem-solving abilities may struggle with managing the unique stressors of college including handling situations when consuming alcohol, such as knowing when to stop drinking, drinking and driving, and binge drinking (Dreer et al., 2004). Williams and Kleinfelter (1989) found that students possessing greater confidence in their problem-solving skills reported less use of alcohol to escape responsibilities and to cope with negative emotions. This finding, which highlights an inverse relationship between increased confidence and decreased alcohol use, supports Bowen’s theory of triangling in order to alleviate emotional overload. McCormack (1996) asked students to indicate if it was acceptable for a student to use alcohol for the following situations: academic pressure, financial problems, family problems, peer pressure, enhance sexual pleasure, at a party, on a date, to relax, and when under stress in general. After comparing this to a 1990 survey with a similar list, the 28 author found an increased percentage (23% to 36%) of students advocated drinking when under stress. Additionally, Lecci, MacLean, and Croteau (2002) found that drinking to cope occurred to help reduce the level of perceived distress toward attaining life goals. Other studies have found small effects (Camatta & Nagoshi, 1995) or no significant relationship (Jones et al., 1992) for alcohol consumption and stress, concluding that college students may be drinking more for socialization than for the alleviation of stress. Specific to the role of triangling alcohol as a means to tolerate anxiety are studies that focus on tension reduction theory (TRT). The premise of TRT is that alcohol is used to reduce stressful states. Kushner et al. (1994) tested the prediction that this theory would be evidenced with a strong correlation between anxiety and alcohol use as they related to alcohol outcome expectancies. Their sample showed females scored significantly higher in interpersonal sensitivity and general anxiety, while males were significantly higher on the quantity/frequency of alcohol consumption and heavy drinking composite. Men and women did not differ in terms of their perceived levels of alcohol tension reduction expectancies. Additionally, males with higher tension reduction expectancies had stronger associations between level of anxiety and alcohol consumption. Similarly, Pullen (1994) sought to find the relationships between alcohol abuse and psychological/demographic variables, including anxiety. The author found that students with high state anxiety abused alcohol more than students with low state anxiety. Similarly, those with high trait anxiety, compared to low trait anxiety, also abused alcohol more significantly. Students who reported abusing alcohol were more likely to be from alcoholic families. Finally, the model that explained 59% of the variance in alcohol 29 abuse contained the following predictive variables: family abuse of alcohol, depression, low levels of self-esteem, state anxiety, assertiveness, and having a lower grade point average. In a longitudinal study of Canadian students, Sadava and Pak (1993) investigated the conditions under which stress can lead to substance abuse. Their findings showed that alcohol use was related to stress, external locus of control, coping functions of drinking, perceived support or sanctions for alcohol use, and the absence of social support. Stewart, Karp, Pihl, and Peterson (1997) conducted two studies that examined students’ use of substances in response to anxiety sensitivity, which was defined as perceiving symptoms of anxiety as signs of catastrophic consequence. The first study focused on substance use in general (non-specific to alcohol) and found female substance users had a positive correlation between anxiety sensitivity scores and the use of alcohol/drugs for reasons related to anxiety, while males also had non-significant positive correlations. The second study focused on specific substances and found a significant positive correlation among female alcohol users, regarding anxiety sensitivity scores and drinking primarily for coping reasons. A similar pattern was found for men, but it did not reach statistical significance. In studying the various expectations students had towards alcohol consumption, Sher, Walitzer, Wood, and Brent (1991) found that men had greater expectations for activity enhancement, performance enhancement, and social lubrication with their drinking than did women. Additionally, COAs reported stronger tension reduction, performance enhancement, and social lubrication expectations than did non-COAs. Moreover, students were found to drink more frequently if they had a greater motivation 30 to drink in order to increase comfort with others and for those who supported drinking in social situations (Williams & Kleinfelter, 1989). Although these studies showed a significant relationship, Noel and Cohen (1997) did not find a significant relationship between stress and alcohol consumption. The authors assessed how college students reacted to a specific time of stress by measuring self-reported substance use during the week before exams and during a typical week in the semester. Students reported drinking a lesser average of Standard Drinks per day during the week before exams (1.06) compared to a typical week (1.48). This decrease contradicts tension reduction theory by illustrating a negative correlation between drinking and anxiety. The authors speculate that studying, which requires sobriety, serves as an effective technique for managing the anxiety related to exam performance. The increasingly large population of college students and their families will undoubtedly face new stressors and anxieties throughout their academic careers. The literature implies individuals with high levels of differentiation are more successful at coping with college adjustment stressors. Johnson and Pandina (2000) found that students who used avoidant strategies in the face of stress had greater levels of alcohol consumption than those with active coping styles. It is crucial to discover how the differentiation process interacts with levels of anxiety and how alcohol is triangled as a dysfunctional coping strategy to assist college students and their families with this life period. Specifically, Goluke, Landeen, and Meadows (1983) detail the processes through which coping to stressors via alcohol can eventually snowball into alcoholism. Although periodic use of alcohol to alleviate stress may not be a risk factor for developing alcoholism, the gateway to addiction may be more 31 prevalent for students with low levels of differentiation, as they may become more reliant on using alcohol as they have a restricted ability to react objectively to stressors. Perceptions of others ’ alcohol use. Multiple studies have illustrated that the perceptions students have towards the amount of alcohol their peers drink is related to the amount that they themselves drink (Clapp & McDonnell, 2000; Demers et al., 2002; Korcuska & Thombs, 2003; Perkins & Berkowitz, 1986; Perkins & Wechsler, 1996). Perkins and Wechsler found that if a student believes normative student consumption of alcohol is in heavy amounts, they are more likely to become involved in alcohol abuse. Yet if a student personally feels very strongly about abstinence or restrained drinking, then perception of other students’ drinking has very little effect. This dynamic relates to Bowen’s notion of having a stable sense-of-self, as students choose behaviors based on prevailing group norms and dogma, rather than from their own convictions. Unfortunately, many students overestimate the amount of alcohol their friends drink (Korcuska & Thombs, 2003; Perkins & Berkowitz, 1986; Perkins & Wechsler, 1996). Additionally, there seems to be a tendency to overestimate the amount of alcohol students consume the more removed the comparison group is from the student. For example, Korcuska and Thombs illustrated that students perceive their same-sex peers to consume more alcohol than they do themselves and that the average same-sex student consumes more than their same-sex peers. An immediate consequence of maintaining this misperception is the greater tendency to consume more alcohol relative to the perceived norm. For example, students that binge drink have been show to report higher perceptions of fellow student alcohol consumption (Dreer et al., 2004). This highlights the notion that even perceptions of one’s environment have a strong ecological influence on individuals. 32 Although there does not appear to be gender-based differences in levels of perceived alcohol consumption (Demers et al., 2002), the effect of having a higher perception related to greater alcohol consumption has been reported to be stronger in men (Korcuska & Thombs, 2003). There is also evidence that drinking behaviors of males and females may be quite similar as there is a stronger association between drinking behaviors to perceived same-sex peer norms than differences in consumption based solely on gender (Fromme & Ruela, 1994). Fromme and Ruela (1994) studied the perceptions and actual alcohol use of undergraduate students along with their parents and fiiends. The authors found that the quantity of alcohol students consumed per drinking occasion was positively correlated to the actual quantity mothers’ consumed per drinking occasion, the students’ perceptions of the mothers’ alcohol consumption, the students’ perceptions of fathers’ alcohol consumption, but not to the actual quantity of alcohol per drinking occasion. Positive correlations were also found between students’ consumption and their friends’ actual quantity of alcohol consumed per drinking occasion, frequency of days consuming alcohol per week, and the total amount of alcohol consumed per week. Similar to other research, positive correlations were also found for students’ actual use and their perceptions of their fiiends’ use for actual quantity of alcohol consumed per drinking occasion, frequency of days consuming alcohol per week, and the total amount of alcohol consumed per week. These findings indicate both direct (actual use) and indirect (perceptions) effects for student alcohol consumption. This suggests that important individuals are modeling alcohol use, and that students’ perceptions regarding alcohol consumption of important others is influential in their own use of alcohol. In terms of 33 Bowen Theory, the perceptions college students have about the normative use of alcohol by family and fiiends may make it more likely that they choose alcohol as a means to diffuse anxiety (a triangle) than some other method (i.e., other illicit substances). Summary Although no studies to date have directly examined the relationship between Bowen’s theory of differentiation and alcohol use, inferences related to some of the fimdamental principles have been demonstrated ill the literature. Evidence has been presented to support the proposed model of study. Additionally, the literature also suggests that effects of COA status (modeling triangle behaviors via alcohol consumption) and perceptions of others’ use (the normative acceptance to triangle alcohol as opposed to other substances) and expectations of alcohol reducing anxiety (triangles) will influence the relationship between differentiation and alcohol consumption and will also provide significant paths in the proposed model. 34 Chapter Three Chapter Three provides conceptual and operational definitions of the variables studied, along with the research objectives and hypotheses that described the predicted relationship of these variables. Additionally, the sampling procedure, data collection, and the measurements used will be described. Methods Conceptual and Operational Definitions Dependent variables. 0 General alcohol consumption Congptualization: The amount of alcoholic drinks generally consumed by the student. gmbnalizafion: This was acquired through a self-report measure. The total amount of general alcohol consumption was the product of the number of times the student drank in the past 30 days and the average amount of alcohol consumed per occasion. One drink was equal to a 12-02 beer, a 4-oz glass of wine, a 12-oz wine cooler, or a 125-02 shot of liquor, either straight or in a mixed drink. 0 Binge drinking Conc_eptualization: Binge drinking is a behavior in which males have 5 or more alcoholic drinks per occasion and females have 4 or more. gmtionalization: This was acquired through self-report. The frequency of binge drinking consisted of the number of times over the past 2 weeks that the student consumed 5 or more (if male, otherwise 4 or 35 more) drinks per occasion. One drink was equal to a 12-02 beer, a 4-02 glass of wine, a 12-oz wine cooler, or a 125-02 shot of liquor, either straight or in a mixed drink. Independent variables. 0 Differentiation Conc_eptualization: An individual's emotional reactivity, ability to take an “1” position, their degree of emotional cutoff from their family-of- origin, and their degree of fusion with others in the face of anxiety/stress. Qm_ra_tionalization: Measured by the Differentiation of Self Inventory- Revised. The DSI-R is a 46 item, six-point Likert scale with increasing scores reflecting higher levels of differentiation. 0 Gender M'onalization: Measured by self-report. 0 Age M'onalization: Measured by self-report. Participation was restricted to those who were between the ages of 18-22. a Ethnicity/Race M'onalization: Measured by self-report. a BMI Comtualization: An index used in the medical community for assessing if an individual is within normal or obese ranges will be used as an estimate for the body’s ability to metabolize alcohol. 36 Omtionalization: Height and weight was measured by self-report. The value of BMI was derived by dividing weight in pounds by height in inches squared and multiplying that product by 703 [BMI = (lb/in2)703] (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2004). Moderator variables. 0 Parental alcoholism Comtualization: The student’s feelings or perceptions that his mother or father was an alcoholic. ngg tionalization: Measured by an investigator created gender-specific version of the Children of Alcoholics Screening Test. Both the mother W-CAST) and father (F -CAST') versions consisted of 29 yes/no questions. The presence of 6 positive responses indicated the presence of an alcoholic parent. 0 Perceptions of normative student drinking Comtualization: The amount of alcohol believed to be consumed by the average student at MSU. M'onalization: This was acquired through self-report. The total amount of general alcohol perceived to be consumed was the product of what the participant believed to be the number of times the average student drank in the past month and the average amount of alcohol the average student consumed per occasion. One drink was equal to a 12- oz beer, a 4-02 glass of wine, a 12-oz wine cooler, or a 125-02 shot of liquor, either straight or in a mixed drink. 37 o Perceptions of best friend’s use Congptualization: The amount of alcohol believed to be consumed by their best friend attending MSU. Qperationalization: This was acquired through self-report. The total amount of general alcohol perceived to be consumed was the product of what the participant believed to be the number of times her best friend at MSU drank in the past month and the average amormt of alcohol consumed per occasion. One drink was equal to a 12-oz beer, a 4-02 glass of wine, a 12-oz wine cooler, or a 125-02 shot of liquor, either straight or in a mixed drink. 0 Expectations of alcohol as a tension reducer Comtualization: The student’s belief that consuming alcohol will relieve tension and anxiety. gmbnalization: The Tension reduction subscale was from Kushner et al. (1998). Higher scores on the 9 item, 5-point Likert scale, indicated greater expectations of tension reduction. 0 Expectations of alcohol as a social lubricant Comafiizafion: The student’s belief that consuming alcohol will aid in social interactions. M'onalization: The social lubricant subscale is from Kushner et al. (1998). Higher scores on the 8 item, S-point Likert scale, indicated greater expectations of social lubrication. 38 Control variables. 0 Traditional student Conggptualization: Participating students will be within the traditional age range of starting college after high school and graduating within five years. M'onalization: As previously mentioned, participation was restricted to those who were between the ages of 18-22. Screened for by listing this as a restriction on the online version and verbally when students were recruited in classrooms for paper-and-pencil versions. 0 Student is a US. citizen Cor_rc_eptualization: The student must be a US. citizen whose family lives in the US. Qaglionalization: Screened for by listing this as a restriction on the online version and verbally when students were recruited in classrooms for paper-and-pencil versions. Research Objectives The overall objective of this research was to investigate the relationship between levels of differentiation and alcohol use for undergraduate students. In order to reach this objective it was necessary to understand the interactions between differentiation and moderating variables. The following research objectives began with the goal of gaining knowledge of the relationship between differentiation and alcohol use and then proceeded with understanding the interaction relationship between differentiation and the potential moderating variables. 39 Diflerentiation and General Alcohol Use and Binge Drinking 1. To identify the relationship with differentiation and general alcohol use and binge drinking. Four Factors of the DSI-R (Emotional Reactivity, “I ” Position, Emotional C utofif And Fusion with Others) with General Alcohol Use and Binge Drinking 2. To identify the relationship of the four factors of the DSI-R with general alcohol and binge drinking. Moderating Eflects on the Relationship between Diflerentiation and General Alcohol Use and Binge Drinking for College Students 3. To identify the moderating effects of COA status on the relationship between differentiation and general alcohol use and binge drinking. 4. To identify the moderating effects of perceptions of the average MSU student’s alcohol use on the relationship between differentiation and general alcohol use and binge drinking. 5. To identify the moderating effects of perceptions of best friend at MSU’s alcohol use on the relationship between differentiation and general alcohol use and binge drinking. 6. To identify the moderating effects of expectations of alcohol serving as a tension reducer on the relationship between differentiation and general alcohol use and binge drinking. 7. To identify the moderating effects of expectations of alcohol serving as a social lubricant on the relationship between differentiation and general alcohol use and binge drinking. Hypotheses The following hypotheses of alcohol consumption were predicted based upon the Bowenian concepts of differentiation and triangling and the literature related to COA status, perceptions of alcohol use, and expectations of alcohol use as a tension reducer or a social lubricant. Difl'erentiation and General Alcohol Use and Binge Drinking Hal: It was expected that college students with lower levels of differentiation would be more likely to have greater amounts of general alcohol and greater amounts of binge drinking consumption when controlling for BMI and age. Four Factors of the DSI-R (Emotional Reactivity, “I " Position, Emotional C utofif and Fusion with Others) with General Alcohol Use and Binge Drinking Ha2: It was expected that college students with lower levels of the four factors of the DSI-R would be more likely to have greater amounts of general alcohol consumption and greater levels of binge drinking when controlling for BMI and age. Further it was predicted that the interpersonal subscales (emotional cutoff and fusion with others) would provide the greatest amount of explained variance in alcohol use for males as it was thought they would have a greater tendency to drink for social reasons. Additionally, it was predicted that the intrapersonal subscales (emotional reactivity and “1” position) would provide the greatest amount of explained variance in 41 alcohol use for females as it was thought they would have a greater tendency to drink due to problem-solving and for coping reasons. Moderating Eflects on the Relationship between Dfirentiation and General Alcohol Use and Binge Drinking Ha3: It was predicted that having an alcoholic parent would have a moderating effect on the relationship between differentiation and general alcohol use and binge drinking. Students that possessed lower levels of differentiation would have greater amounts of alcohol consumption to the extent that they had a greater number of alcoholic parents. It was thought COAs would have a history of parents modeling alcohol use as a triangle to tolerate anxiety. Ha4: Perceptions of the average MSU student’s alcohol use would have a moderating effect on the relationship between differentiation and general alcohol use and binge drinking. Students who had lower levels of differentiation would have greater amounts of alcohol consumption to the extent that they had greater amounts of perceived student use, presenting as a normative means by which to diffuse anxiety. Ha5: Perceptions of student’s best friend at MSU’s alcohol use would have a moderating effect on the relationship between differentiation and general alcohol use and binge drinking. Students who had lower levels of differentiation would have greater amounts of alcohol consumption to the extent that they had greater amounts of perceived best fiiend use, presenting as a normative means to diffuse anxiety. 42 Ha6: Expectations of alcohol serving as a tension reducer would have a moderating effect on the relationship between differentiation and general alcohol use and binge drinking. Students who had lower levels of differentiation would have greater amounts of alcohol consumption to the extent that they had higher expectations that alcohol would alleviate anxiety. Ha7: Expectations of alcohol serving as a social lubricant would have a moderating efl‘ect on the relationship between differentiation and general alcohol use and binge drinking. Students who had lower levels of differentiation would have greater amounts of alcohol consumption to the extent that they had higher expectations that alcohol would serve as a social lubricant as a means to help tolerate relational anxiety. Decision Rule: A chance probability of .05 or less (p < .05) was required to reject the null hypotheses. Research Design Individual undergraduate college students were the unit of analysis for this cross- sectional, survey research. The dependent variables studied were college student alcohol consumption (averaged Quantity X Frequency over a 30 day period) and frequency of binge drinking (over a 2 week period). A minimum of 500 students were to be sampled as this number would exceed Cohen’s (1992) criteria for a medium effect size with an alpha value of .05 if there were a need to analyze males and females separately. 43 Measures Dfirentiation The Differentiation of Self Inventory-Revised (DSI-R) measured the degree of differentiation for college students (see Appendix A). The DSI-R was chosen as it is one of the few self-report scales that broadly cover the construct of differentiation. The original DSI scale (Skowron & Friedlander, 1998) was a 43 item, six-point Likert instrument consisting of four subscales: Emotional Reactivity (ER), 1 Position (1P), Emotional Cutoff (EC), and Fusion with Others (F0). Estimates of internal consistency reliability using Cronbach’s alpha yielded high values for the D81 full scale (a = .88) and each ofthe subscales (ER or = .84, IP or = .83, BC or = .82, F0 or = .74). The DSI was revised to improve the psychometrics of the Fusion with Others (F O) subscale (Skowron & Schmitt, 2003). The revised FO scale changed from 9 items to 12, resulting in a 46 item scale. The internal consistency reliabilities improved for both the F O subscale (a = .86) and the DSI—R full-scale (a = .92). Each item in the DSI-R presents a 6-point Likert scale ranging from “not at all true of me” to “very true of me.” After reversing the responses on select questions, scores on the subscales and total DSI-R are obtained by summing the respective raw scores and dividing by the total number of items per scale. For example, the DSI-R full-scale is obtained by summing all of the values and dividing by 46, with scores reflect a range of 1 (low differentiation) to 6 (higher differentiation). The D81 and DSI—R were originally tested with a sample that was 25-years old and older. After analyzing the four-factor structure with the college students in this study, three of the items (#11 fiom the “1” Position subscale, #32 from the Emotional Cutoff subscale, and #38 hen) the Emotional Reactivity subscale), were omitted in order to maintain a significant fora-factor structure for both males and females when analyzed separately (see Appendix B for detailed analysis of the DSI-R with college students). The full-scale and subscale Cronbach’s alpha values remained similar to the earlier studies (ER 0. = .86; IP or = .77; BC or = .80; F0 or = .69; DSI-R or = .90), with the exception of the FO subscale which was .17 lower than the Skowron & Schmitt (2003) sample. Parental History of A lcohol Use A minimally altered version of the Children of Alcoholics Screening Test (CAST) (see Appendix C for the original CAST) was used to assess parental alcoholism. The CAST is a 30—item instrument that asks about perceptions regarding parental alcohol consumption. The total number of questions to which subjects reply “yes,” yields a total score that can range from 0 to 30. Scores of six or more indicate the subject is likely the child of an alcoholic (Jones, 1983; Kelly & Myers, 1996; Yeatman et al., 1994). The CAST has demonstrated high alpha coefficients with studies of randomly selected adolescents (0.95 - males, 0.97 - females, and 0.96 - combined gender, Dinning & Berk, 1989) as well as for adolescents who were selected from intact alcoholic families (0.90 and 0.88, Clair & Genest, 1992). Charland and Cdté (1992) found an extremely high test- retest reliability (k = .83) and concurrent validity with the Structured Clinical Interview for the DSM—III-R (SCID) and high CAST scores (k = .78). Additionally, Charland and Cété’s study with 376 college students illustrated that the CAST yielded a false-negative rate of 9.3% and a false-positive rate of 1.2%. In a study of adolescent offspring of alcoholic fathers, Clair and Genest (2002) found test-retest reliability coefficient of .88 after a period of8 weeks. 45 As this study is interested in examining the effects of the number of alcoholic parents, the CAST was rewarded to create a specific version for the mother (M-CAST, see Appendix D) and the father (F-CAST, see Appendix E). The revised inventory replaced the CAST’s terminology of “a parent” used throughout the inventory to “mother” and “father” respectively. There are two questions on the CAST related to specific parent use (“Did you ever think your father was an alcoholic?”/“Did you ever think your mother was an alcoholic?"). The question for the father was omitted on the M- CAST and the question for the mother was omitted on the F -CAST. The creation of separate inventories afforded a more specific analysis for perceptions in which the mother, the father, or both parents had a history with alcoholism. The total number of alcoholic parents (0, 1, or 2) was indicated by the presence of six positive responses on the M- and F -CASTs. General Alcohol Use Subjects were asked about their alcohol use over the past month in terms of fiequency and quantity (see Appendix F, questions 18 and 19). Subjects reported how many times they drank in the past 30 days and the average amount of drinks they consumed per occasion. One drink was defined as a 12-02 beer, a 4-oz glass of wine, a 12-oz wine cooler, or a 125-02 shot of liquor, either straight or in a mixed drink. A total average of drinks-per—month was calculated by multiplying the number of days by the average number of drinks. Binge Drinking Male subjects were asked on how many occasions over the past 2 weeks they consumed 5 or more beverages, while females were asked on how many occasions they 46 consumed 4 or more beverages (see Appendix F, questions 20 and 21). These numbers reflected criteria for binge drinking according to Wechsler, Lee, Nelson, et al. (1991). Expectancies of Alcohol as a Tension Reducer and Social Lubricant Kushner et al.’s (1994) measure of alcohol outcome expectancies was used to evaluate motivation for drinking (see Appendix G). Sher et al. created the scale after being dissatisfied with other expectation measures that solely used dichotomous criteria by including questions fi'om various measures into 12 a priori domains and running a factor analysis to streamline their scale. The current scale consists of 35 questions using a 5 -point scale that load on the following four factors: Tension Reduction (9 items; questions 1-9), Social Lubrication (8 items; questions 10-17), Performance Enhancement (9 items), and Activity Enhancement (9 items), where higher sums of scores reflect greater prevalence of respective expectations. The subscales have a common variance ranging from .54 to .70 (Kushner et al., 1994). For the purposes of this study, the Tension Reduction (a = .89) and Social Lubrication (a = .88) subscales were used in the analyses. Perception of the Average Michigan State Student 's Alcohol Use Subjects were asked how much alcohol over the past month in terms of fiequency and quantity they believed the average Michigan State student consumed (see Appendix F, questions 24 and 25). A total average of drinks-per-month was calculated by multiplying the number of days by the average number of drinks. Perception of Their Best Friend at Michigan State 's Alcohol Use Subjects were asked about how much alcohol over the past month in terms of fiequency and quantity they believed their best fiiend at Michigan State consumed (see 47 Appendix E, questions 28 and 29). A total average of drinkscper-month was calculated by multiplying the number of days by the average number of drinks. Demographic Information In addition to the measurements, subjects completed a demographic sheet recording their gender, age, race, grade level, body mass index, and living location (see Appendix F). Sample It was originally proposed that 500 subjects would be recruited fi'om the Department of Psychology’s Human Subjects P001 to voluntarily complete the battery online for extra credit in their respective courses. Due to a high number of studies taking place during the semester, a recruiting restriction was set by the department and permission was given to recruit 162 subjects. In order to meet Cohen’s (1992) criteria for a medium effect size, undergraduate students were then recruited from Family and Child Ecology (F CE) undergraduate courses to anonymously complete paper-and-pencil versions of the battery. Students were instructed that they could only complete the measme once, so that if they had completed it from another class they were told not to complete another one. As a recruiting incentive, six of the paper-and-pencil subjects were randomly selected to win one of six $50 prizes. After recruiting subjects from the Human Subjects Pool and F CE courses, there was a shortage of males so additional subjects were recruited fi'om two courses in the Department of Computer Science. At the same time as the Computer Science students were being recruited, the restrictions imposed by the Human Subjects Pool were lified and additional subjects were recruited online. All subjects read and agreed with the consent form prior to participation (see Appendix H). 48 Additionally, debriefing information regarding the nature of the study and instructions for contacting the researcher and the University Committee on Research Involving Human Subjects was provided (see Appendix 1). Responses to all measures were anonymous. The proposal to University Committee on Research Involving Human Subjects (UCRIHS) stated that efforts would be made to keep the total number of subjects recruited to be around the range of 500 students. The data from the Computer Science courses were never entered into the data set as they were the last to be recruited and their paper-and-pencil forms were returned after the total number of subjects was extending beyond the target range. Although the student responses were not entered into the data set, their names were entered into the $50 prize lottery. All data collected from the PCB courses were entered into the data set. Since it was a goal to recruit an approximately equal number of males and females, the second wave of females recruited from the Human Subjects Pool were omitted from the data set as the maximum number of female subject had already been reached. A total of 447 subjects (246 females, 20] males) were analyzed for this study after omitting 39 subjects for missing data, 23 for having impossible or incongruent information (e. g., what they reported their gender to be differed from the gender-specific question related to binge drinking, or 17 days of binge drinking over the past 2 weeks), and 13 for being outside of the l8-22-year old age range, The final sample exceeded Cohen’s (1992) criteria for a medium effect size. A total of 256 individuals (102 females, 154 males) completed the online version of the battery via the Human Subjects Pool and 191 students (144 females, 47 males) from six FCE courses completed the paper-and- pencil version of the battery. 49 Data Analysis Once data entry into SPSS 13.0 was completed, the data were cleaned making sure all values were within the appropriate ranges, omitting cases for missing data, and examining outliers prior to running analyses. Examination of the frequency distribution of general alcohol use (GAU) and binge drinking outcome variables indicated the data were both positively skewed and were subsequently transformed into categorical variables (see Appendix J). Males and females were tested for differences in alcohol consumption with the respective categories. The chi-square test of association indicated that males and females were different in binge drinking behavior and therefore were analyzed separately for the remaining analyses in order to avoid interpreting possible three-way interaction effects (see Appendix K). All of the variables were centered around their means prior to running the regressions in order to minimize multicollinearity. Following the variance inflation factor protocol (von Eye & Schuster, 1998), diagnosis for multicollinearity was implemented prior to all regression analyses by regressing all of the predictors in the respective models onto each other (see Appendix K). There were no issues of multicollinearity discovered between the variables. The hypotheses were tested through a series bf logistic regression analyses. In ' order for the difl‘erentiation and four factor models to be considered significant, the overall model had to be significant (p < .05, for example see Equation 1) and the respective variables had to significantly alter the odds of drinking classification (Wald statistic was significant at p < .05), regardless of the magnitude of the odds ratio. As a 50 guide for the magnitude of effect size, Hopkins (2002) reports that an odds ratio of 1.50 is considered small, 3.5 is moderate and 9.0 is large. GAU = b0 + erifferentiation + szMI + b3Age (1) When testing the moderating hypotheses the overall models and individual interaction terms had to be significant. Additionally, in order to preserve the most parsimonious model, the Interaction Model (for example see Equation 3) had to show a significant block improvement over the Additive Model (for example see Equation 2) or the Basic Model (if the Additive Model did not show a significant improvement over the Basic Model) to justify adding additional variables. GAU = b0 + erifferentiation + szMI + b3Age + b4COA (2) GAU = bo + leifferentiation+ szMI + bgAge + b4COA + b5(Differentiation X COA) (3) Logistic regression does not possess an equivalent to the OLS R2. Although, pseudo R2 estimates are available, they do not describe the proportion of variance explained by the predictors nor do they compare directly to other R2 measures. The Cox and Snell R2 (RICO, a 5...") and Nagelkerke’s R2 (RZNWC) are provided. The RIC“ a 5,... attempts to create a value similar to the OLS R2 but the maximum range is typically below 1, while 51 the Rzngem“, manipulates the R20,x a 3.3.. so that the value ranges from 0 to 1. Both R2 are provided for the significant analyses. Chapter Four presents the results of the logistic regressions as they relate to the respective hypotheses. Complete analyses, including non-significant results, can be found in Appendices L and M. 52 Chapter Four This chapter presents the results of the logistic regression analyses for general alcohol use (GAU) and binge drinking as they relate to the specific hypotheses. Only results specifically related to the hypotheses are presented. For a more detailed presentation of the logistic regressions, including the main effects found in the Additive Models, see Appendix L for GAU analyses and Appendix M for binge drinking analyses. Analyses of General Alcohol Use As previously described, the continuous General Alcohol Use (GAU) measure (30 day Quantity X Frequency) was converted into a categorical variable consisting of 4 levels: Abstainers, Low Drinkers, Moderate Drinkers, and High Drinkers (see Appendix J). As the four categories reflect increasing degrees of alcohol consumption, the nature of this study’s originally stated hypotheses for a continuous dependent variable (e. g., students with lower levels of differentiation will be more likely to have greater amounts of general alcohol consumption) was maintained with the categorical analyses (e.g., students with lower levels of differentiation will be more likely to have a greater relationship with High Drinkers than Low Drinkers when controlling for BMI and age). . As logistic regression is based on a dichotomous dependent variable, the four GAU categories were contrasted in ways such that four tmique outcome dichotomies were created: 1. Drinkers (Low, Moderate, High) versus Abstainers 2. Moderate Drinkers versus Low Drinkers 3. High Drinkers versus Low Drinkers 4. High Drinkers versus Moderate Drinkers 53 In each instance, the variable with the greater amount of alcohol consumption was coded as a “1” in SPSS 13.0 and the lesser category was coded as a “0.” For example, in the Drinkers versus Abstainers analyses, all persons in the Low, Moderate, and High Drinking categories were coded as a “1” and Abstainers were coded as a “0.” The analyses began by testing the hypotheses of differentiation and the four-factor model on levels of alcohol consumption for each of the four dichotomies. The regression equation that consists of differentiation, age, and BMI is referred to as the Basic Model (see Equation 1, p. 51). The second step was to simply add the moderating variable of interest to the regression equation, creating the Additive Model (see Equation 2, p. 51). Thirdly, the Interaction Model was tested by adding the interaction term to the regression equation (see Equation 3, p. 51). The moderating hypotheses were then tested with each of the dichotomies with the criteria that the overall models and individual interaction terms had to be significant. Additionally, in order to preserve the most parsimonious model, the Interaction Model had to show a significant block improvement over the Additive Model (simply controlling for the added variable) or the Basic Model (if the Additive Model did not show a significant improvement over the Basic Model) to justify adding additional variables. Finally, a Comprehensive Model was created by adding all of the variables into one regression equation which was compared to the Basic Model for a significant block improvement. Dlfl'erentiation and GA U It was hypothesized that college students with lower levels of differentiation would be more likely to have greater amounts of general alcohol consumption when controlling for BMI and age. Males. There were differing levels of support for the hypotheses across the drinking category comparisons. In the Drinkers versus Abstainers category the Basic Model was significant (38(3) = 18.00, p < .001, 12%,, g 3,... = .09, mm“. = .20). Differentiation had a significant negative coefficient indicating that the odds of drinking were multiplied by .190 (95% CI = .068, .530) with a one unit difference in differentiation -- an 81% (l - .19) decrease. As differentiation levels increased the likelihood these male students would drink decreased, which was predicted in the hypothesis. Additionally, age was significant as students were more likely to drink as they got older. The hypothesis was not supported in the comparisons of Moderate versus Low Drinkers (38(3) = 2.92, p = .405), High versus Low Drinkers (36(3) = 2.21, p = .530), and High versus Moderate Drinkers (38(3) = 5.35 p = .119) as the Basic Model failed to reach a level of significance. Females. The differentiation hypothesis was not supported in any of the comparisons for females. All of the models failed to reach a level of significance: Drinkers versus Abstainers (£0) = 6.91, p = .08), Moderate versus Low Drinkers (12(3) = 2.62, p = .45), High versus Low Drinkers (36(3) = 2.06, p = .56), and High versus Moderate Drinkers (38(3) = 4.10, p = .25). However, although the overall model was not significant in relation to the population, differentiation was a significant predictor in changing the odds of being a Drinker versus Abstainer for the sample by .510 (95% CI = .268, .970) for every one unit increase in differentiation. 55 F our-F actors of the DSI-R with GA U It was hypothesized that college students with lower levels of the four factors of the DSI-R (Emotional Reactivity, “1” Position, Emotional Cutoff, and Fusion with Others) would be more likely to have greater amounts of general alcohol consumption when controlling for BMI and age. It was also predicted that the interpersonal subscales (emotional cutoff and fusion with others) would provide the greatest amount of explained variance in general alcohol use for males as there would be a greater tendency to drink for social reasons. Additionally, it was predicted that the intrapersonal subscales (emotional reactivity and “1” position) would provide the greatest amount of explained variance in general alcohol use for females as there would be a greater tendency to drink due to problem solving and coping reasons. Males. There were difl'ering levels of support for the four-factor hypotheses across the drinking category comparisons. The F our-F actor Model was supported in the High versus Moderate Drinker comparison (38(6) = 16.35, p = .012, Ric,“ M= .13, RZNM = .17). Although the interpersonal subscales (emotional cutoff and fusion with others) were predicted to provide the greatest contribution for male drinking; the intrapersonal subscales (emotional reactivity and “1” position) emerged as the significant individual predictors. A one unit increase in Emotional Reactivity (being less emotionally reactive) increased the likelihood of being a High Drinker (OR = 1.928, 95% CI = .999, 3.720), whereas increasing the “1” Position value by one unit decreased the likelihood of being a High versus a Moderate Drinker (OR = .374, 95% CI = .198, .706). For males it appears that maintaining a sense-of-self (“1” Position) made it less likely that one would 56 escalate into the High Drinking category, while remaining less emotionally reactive to stress increased the likelihood of being a High versus a Moderate Drinker. The overall models were not significant for the Moderate versus Low Drinkers or the High versus Low Drinkers comparisons. Although the Drinkers versus Abstainers comparison was significant as an overall model, none of the four factors significantly changed the odds of group membership. Females. The four-factor hypothesis was not supported in any of the comparisons. Each of the models failed to reach a level of significance: Drinkers versus Abstainers (38(6) = 9.21, p = .162), Moderate versus Low Drinkers (38(6) = 2.97, p = .813), High versus Low Drinkers (£0) = 3.41, p = .757), and High versus Moderate Drinkers (x2(6) = 4.49, p = .610). Moderator Hypotheses It was hypothesized that students with lower levels of differentiation would have greater amounts of general alcohol use to the extent that they had greater perceptions of others’ use (the average MSU student’s use and best friend at MSU’s use), greater expectations of alcohol as a tension reducer and social lubricant, and if they were a child of an alcoholic (COA). The male or female analyses failed to provide support for the perceptions of best fiiend at MSU’s use, COA status, tension reduction, or social lubricant interaction hypotheses as the interaction term failed to reach a level of significance across all of the comparison groups. The perception of MSU student use was partially supported by both the males and females. Males. The High versus Moderate Drinkers comparison did provide support for the interaction hypothesis as the Interaction Model was significant (xz(5) = 13.21, p = 57 .022, RICO, g 5.,“ = .10, Rznwgk, = .14) and showed a block improvement over the Basic Model (38(2) = 7.36, p = .025), as the Additive Model failed to reach significance. The interaction of differentiation and perception of MSU student use was a significant individual predictor with individuals being .978 (95% CI = .957, .999) times as likely to be a High Drinker- versus a Moderate Drinker (see Table 1 and Figure 3, p. 60, for illustration of the interaction effect). Subjects with a standard deviation difference (20.25) of the interaction were .63 times as likely to be a High Drinker versus a Moderate Drinker. The overall Interaction Model failed to reach a level of significance for the Moderate to Low Drinkers model. Although the overall models were significant for the Drinkers versus Abstainers and the High versus Low Drinkers comparisons, neither had a significant block improvement over the Additive Model nor had a significant interaction term. Table 1 Logistic Regression Predicting Male Students that are High versus Moderate Drinkers - Perceptions of MSU Student Use Interaction Model b SE Wald OR 95% CI DSI—R -0.23 0.40 0.34 0.798 0.375 1.699 BMI 0.11 0.07 2.28 1.116 0.968 1.288 Age 0.12 0.16 0.56 1.124 0.827 1.528 MSU Use 0.01 0.01 1.43 1.006 0.996 1.017 DSI-R X MSU Use -0.02 0.01 4.23“ 0.978 0.957 0.999 * p < .05, two-tailed. ** p < .01, two-tailed. Females. The High versus Low Drinkers comparison was significant as an overall model (38(5) =' 18.01, p = .003, RICOXgM= .11, Rimm = .15) and had a significant block improvement over the Additive Model (38(1) = 4.59, p = .032). The interaction of 58 differentiation with perception of MSU student use was significant with an odds ratio of .983 (95% CI = .967, .999) for every unit increase in the interaction term (see Table 2 and see Figure 4, p. 62, for illustration of the interaction effect). Subjects with a standard deviation difference (21.57) of the interaction were .65 times as likely to be High versus Low Drinkers. The overall Interaction Model failed to reach a level of significance for the Drinkers versus Abstainers and Moderate versus Low Drinkers comparisons. Although the Interaction Model was significant as an overall model in the High versus Moderate Drinkers comparison, the interaction term failed to reach a level of significance and the model did not demonstrate a significant block improvement over the Basic Model. Table 2 Logistic Regression Predicting Female Students that are High versus Low Drinkers — Perceptions of MSU Student Use [ b 1 SE [ Wald [ 0R 1 95% CI Interaction Model DSI-R -0.03 0.30 0.01 0.968 0.539 1.741 BMI -005 0.05 0.89 0.951 0.858 1.055 Age 0.05 0.14 0.14 1.056 0.797 1.398 MSU Use 0.02 0.01 9.85” 1.015 1.006 1.025 DSI-R x MSU Use 0012 0.01 434* 0.983 0.967 0.999 " p < .05, two-tailed. *" p < .01, two-tailed. 59 ,_____, .‘ ...,- - - MSU Use—High ’ i — + - MSU Use Average * —-— MSU UsefiLow Predicted Probability of Drinking Category Low Average High Differentiation Figure 3. The differentiation and perception of MSU student alcohol use interaction for the comparison of male High versus Moderate Drinkers. Note: Predicted probability scores > .50 indicate a greater probability for being a High Drinker and < .50 indicate a greater probability for being a Moderate Drinker. Values are when controlling for BMI and age. The Comprehensive Model All of the variables of interest were entered simultaneously into a logistic regression in order to identify significant contributions of individual variables while controlling the influence of the remaining variables. All of the Comprehensive Models showed a significant improvement over the Basic Model. Males. Each of the comprehensive models was significant: Drinkers versus Abstainers (38(13) = 66.18, p < .001, 8%.... 5.... = .28, 192.4%“... = .66), Moderate versus Low Drinkers (38(13) = 34.99, p = .001, 8%.... 5,... = .24, 18......“ = .33), High versus Low Drinkers (38(13) = 86.95, p < .001, 8%.... 3.... = .51, mm... = .68), High versus Moderate Drinkers (38(13) = 51.44, p < .001, 18c... 5.... = .34, 82...“... = .46) (see Table 3, p. 64). Interestingly, there were no individual predictors in this model that significantly changed the odds of being a Drinker versus Abstainer. Perceptions of best fiiend use (OR = 1.020) and expectations of tension reduction (OR = 1.109) significantly improve the odds of being classified as a Moderate Drinker versus a Low Drinker. Perception of best fiiend usage improved the likelihood of being classified as a high drinker (OR = 1.037) while being a COA actually decreased the likelihood of being a High Drinker versus Low Drinker (OR = .188). Perception of best fiiend use was the only significant individual variable in the comparison of High versus Moderate Drinkers (OR = 1.026). 61 0.8 —. - .- __.__n _ Lb- or '5 e «g 0.7 —»——~—~ Jae-iff- ——-e e “n .2 w 3 0.6 Lani. __,____m_ a ~~.., e e 0'5 ‘F‘T' -‘=*<-_-’—‘E::::r-r— '7‘ won-MSU Use High 3 ‘= o . . § 5’ 0-4 ‘F -*——~ —~---~ ~ i-f-MSU Use Average: ‘2: 8 0 3 _ ___ g—I——MSU Use Low 11. '/ — — ~ 3 02 — ____- 4 ~ — —_ .4 _ ii 0.1 e——-- —~~ ~ «— IL Ol-—————e._.___.-__, Low Average High Differentiation Figure 4. The differentiation and perception of MSU student alcohol use interaction for female High versus Low Drinkers. Note: Predicted probability scores > .50 indicate a greater probability for being a High Drinker and < .50 indicate a greater probability for being a Low Drinker. Values are when controlling for BMI and age. 62 Females. The Comprehensive Model was significant for each of the female comparisons: Drinkers versus Abstainers (38(13) = 90.70, p < .001, RIC... .. 5.... = .31, alum = .57), Moderate versus Low Drinkers (38(13) = 45.72, p < .001, Ric... 5,... = .28, 82......” = .38), High versus Low Drinkers (38(13) = 88.84, p < .001, 8%., g 5,... = .45, 1?sz = .60), High versus Moderate Drinkers (38(13) = 23.41, p = .037, 82.... .. 5...... = .16, Rznmm = .21) (see Table 4, p. 65). Only the tension reduction variable showed an individual level of significance in changing the odds of being a Drinker versus Abstainer (OR = 1.237). Age (OR = 1.461), perceptions of best friend use (OR = 1.028), and expectations of tension reduction (OR = 1.118) were significant in changing the likelihood of being classified as a Moderate versus Low Drinker. Perceptions of MSU student use (OR = 1.016), perceptions of best friend use (OR = 1.048), and the interaction between differentiation and the perception of best fiiend use (OR = .970, see Figure 5, p. 66) and tension reduction (OR = 1.135) were significant for the High versus Low Drinker comparison. No individual predictors in this model significantly changed the odds of being a High Drinker versus a Moderate Drinker. 63 Hugo w Ooaearoammfin Kean; men Kan Q>C nefiomonam 01388 8 38038 Unis—w Em... Una—83 8 Em: 01383 8 >cm§=na 8 has Dan—83 rose 01383 £9338 Una—88 gag OW $35 0% gig OW $35 ON 02% rue Noam o3 rune o.oo o.ooo o.oo oxfio wZ: o.oo o.oo; one o.ooo 93 926 NS :3 >mo Pom n; 3 o.oo o.omu o.oo fowo P: o.ooo ch enaooeaosm 9: o.ooo NB _.o_~ Ne: roww o.oo o.ooo OMEN X ch eonoooaoam o.on o.ooo obo o.ooo o.om o.oom o.oo o.ooo mom. manna o.: o.ooo flaws... o.ouo No.8... _._ rowu 5.3... a. o.oNo Um?” X mom" 113a o.oo o.ooa o.~o o.ooo rum Bo; ohm o.oow 00> o.oo o.ooo o.ow o.ooo #5... 95¢ who o.ooo UmEN X 00> o.oo N30 95 ream o.om PNE row was H038: x2330: o.oo rmoo 93... :8 Now Co.» o.oo rowu Um?” X H0390: ”mucosa: o.oo _.~ou on: 93¢ 98 o.ooo o.oo : G moor: Economic: N3 Zoe o.oo o.ooo o.oo o.omm o.oo o.ooo Um?” X meow: reggae: o.oo _.oom o.oo row— 98 fowo ohm obs: a. .e A .om. "gram—on. a... a A .or gem—=09. A020 A 0033030380 7888 8.. 330.0 O>C nefiomoag 9.8.83 8 78838 9.8.88 2mm: 9.8.88 8 Em: 9.8.88 8 2008803 8 ~82 Unified. hos. 01383 32.088 01383 $08 0w $88 0% (<08 0% £05 OX Om?” _.oo o. A oo 98 o.ouo o. _ o ..~u~ o. _ A :3 95 o.oA o.oo. o.uo o.omo o._A o.ooo 93 9vo >m0 ouw Zmo A. A we _.Ao_ o. A N room o.oo o.oow ch $80383 o.oo o.ooo o.oo rooo who... :vo row o.ooo Um?” X ch $80388 o.oo room :A o.ooo Nam o.omw foo o.omA mg» m. 103 Poo _.o_ m o.oo: #on No.3: _.oAm Now _.ooo Um?” X w0m. 3.88 93 _.ooo o.A~ o.ooo Aha... o.ooo obo room 00> om» o.ooo o.oo o.ooo o. _o o.og ohm o.ooo Um?” X 00> o.oo :3 PAN room PAN #on o.oo o.ooo H038: ”0828: 93...... Emma ohms... _._ 3 Saws... :3 Now Foam 09% X H038: ”0898: PS _.oo.~ “Co o.ooo o.oo o.omu foo rom— moomfl 581038: o.oo :AN o.oo fou— o.~o o.oNA o.o_ row— Umzw X 808. C8188: o.oo :om row :3 PS rouse o.oo o.vo _.. e A .3. gods—0a. are A .3. 30.5.0? 65 2' g 0 9 ,E__ __.___\_;: --r ‘ if: i _ é.‘; _ ___,:_ __, E 0.8 ‘T— \\ . . '5 0.7 4%»: r ._ .___.\_ mm * - " ‘ r r + a- My _---- ____ ___. z. a, 0.6 4“” ,__._ _,__ _ _ .-__\‘___ __.____n__ . W ' - - -Q- . - BF U88 High - E O a ---- 1‘ ' '8 3 05 ll_____,,_ WW—W—— _ 4 *——~ _ —+-BF Use Average' g 8 0.4 LN: A A __ -_ _________.._ v -- -~ ~ ‘—I—BF Use LOW n. #5. _-_.,__ _- _ _ -- i g T a . Low Average High Differentiation Figure 5. The differentiation and perception of best friend at MSU’s alcohol use interaction for female High versus Low Drinkers. Note: Predicted probability scores > .50 indicate a greater probability for being a High Drinker and < .50 indicate a greater probability for being a Low Drinker. Values are when controlling for all other variables in the Comprehensive Model. Analyses of Binge Drinking As was done with the General Alcohol Use measure, the continuous Binge Drinking variable (number of days in the past 14 days the subject drank at binge levels - - 5 drinks per occasion for males, 4 drinks per occasion for females) was converted into a categorical variable consisting of 4 levels of use: Abstainers (those that did not drink), Nonbinging Drinkers (subjects that consumed alcohol, but not at binge levels), Occassional Binge Drinkers (l or 2 times in the past 2 weeks), and Frequent Binge Drinkers (3 or more times in the past 2 weeks) (see Appendix J). As the four categories reflect increasing degrees of alcohol consumption, the nature of this study’s hypotheses which were stated for a continuous dependent variable (e. g., students’ lower levels of differentiation will be more likely to have greater amounts of general alcohol consumption) was maintained with the categorical analyses (e.g., students’ lower levels of differentiation will be more likely to have a greater relationship with High Drinkers than Low Drinkers when controlling for BMI and age). The four General Alcohol Use categories were contrasted in ways such that four unique outcome dichotomies were created: 1. Binge Drinkers (Occasional & Frequent Bingers) versus Nonbinging Drinkers 2. Occasional Binge Drinkers versus Nonbinging Drinkers 3. Frequent Binge Drinkers versus Nonbinging Binge Drinkers 4. Frequent Binge Drinkers versus Occasional Binge Drinkers The identical coding procedure and process of analysis from the General Alcohol Use tests were implemented with the Binge Drinking analyses. 67 Differentiation and Binge Drinking It was hypothesized that college students with lower levels of differentiation would be more likely to have greater amounts of binge drinking when controlling for BMI and age. Males. The differentiation hypothesis was not supported in any of the comparisons. All of the Basic Models failed to reach a level of significance: Binge Drinker versus Nonbinging Drinkers (£6) = .75, p = .861), Occasional Binge Drinkers versus Nonbinging Drinkers (x20) = .50, p = .929), Frequent Binge Drinkers versus Nonbinging Drinkers (12(3) = 2.48, p = .479), Frequent Binge Drinkers versus Occasional Binge Drinkers (76(3) = 6.83, p = .078). Females. The differentiation hypothesis was not supported in any of the comparisons. All of the Basic Models failed to reach a level of significance: Binge Drinker versus Nonbinging Drinkers (36(3) = 3.38, p = .337), Occasional Binge Drinkers versus Nonbinging Drinkers (12(3) = 1.40, p = .705), Frequent Binge Drinkers versus Nonbinging Drinkers (x20) = 5.81, p = .121), Frequent Binge Drinkers versus Occasional Binge Drinkers (38(3) = 4.59, p = .205). F our—F actors of the DSI-R with Binge Drinking It was hypothesized that college students with lower levels of the four factors of theDSI-R (Emotional Reactivity, “1” Position, Emotional Cutoff, and Fusion with Others) would be more likely to have greater amounts of binge drinking when controlling for BMI and age. Further it was predicted that the interpersonal subscales (emotional cutoff and fusion with others) would provide the greatest amount of explained variance in binge drinking for males as there would be a greater tendency to drink for social reasons. 68 Additionally, it was predicted that the intrapersonal subscales (emotional reactivity and “1” position) would provide the greatest amount of explained variance in binge drinking for females as there would be a greater tendency to drink due to problem solving and coping reasons. Males. The four-factor hypothesis was partially supported in the binge drinking comparisons for males. The F our-F actor Model was supported in the Frequent versus Occasional Binge Drinkers comparison (38(6) = 15.82, p = .015, R20», & 5...." = .10, Rszlkm = .13). Interestingly, it was predicted the interpersonal subscales (emotional cutoff and fusion with others) would provide the greatest contribution for male drinking; however, the “1” Position subscale was the only significant predictor of binge drinking categories, while the Emotional Cut-off subscale approached significance (p = .056). Students who had a one unit higher “1” Position score were .488 (95% CI = .268, .889) times more likely to be Frequent Binge Drinkers versus Occasional Binge Drinkers (see Table 5). This finding indicates that as students become more competent in the development of an “1” position, they are less likely to be Frequent Binge drinkers in comparison to Occasional Binge Drinkers. Table 5 Logistic Regression Predicting Male Students that are Frequent Bingers versus Occasional Bingers — Four Factors b SE Wald OR 95% CI Emotional Reactivity 0.42 0.31 1.91 1.524 0.839 2.769 “I” Position -0.72 0.31 5.50“ 0.488 0.268 0.889 Emotional Cutoff -0.50 0.26 3.67 0.606 0.363 1.012 Fusion with Others -0. 14 0.41 0.12 0.868 0.389 1.937 BMI -0.05 0.05 0.96 0.952 0.862 1 .05 1 Age 0.20 0.15 1.91 1.222 0.920 1.623 * p < .05, two-tailed. ** p < .01, two-tailed. 69 The overall model was not significant for the Binge Drinker versus Nonbinging Drinkers (x2(6) = 1.327, p = .970), Occasional Binge Drinkers versus Nonbinging Drinkers (x2(6) = 2.77, p = .838), Frequent Binge Drinkers versus Nonbinging Drinkers comparisons (12(6) = 3.78, p = .706). Females. The four-factor hypothesis was not supported in any of the comparisons. All of the Four-F actor Models failed to reach a level of significance: Binge Drinker versus Nonbinging Drinkers (x2(6) = 4.99, p = .546), Occasional Binge Drinker versus Nonbinging Drinkers (12(6) = 4.58, p = .599), Frequent Binge Drinkers versus Nonbinging Drinkers (38(6) = 7.39, p = .286), and Frequent Binge Drinkers versus Occasional Binge Drinkers (36(6) = 9.05, p = .171). Moderator Hypotheses It was hypothesized that the students with lower levels of differentiation would have greater amounts of binge drinking to the extent that they had greater perceptions of others use (the average MSU student use and best friend at MSU’s use), greater expectations of alcohol as a tension reducer and social lubricant, and they were COAs. The male or female analyses failed to provide support for the perceptions of best friend at MSU’s use, tension reduction, or social lubricant interaction hypotheses as the interaction term failed to reach a level of significance across all of the comparison groups. Although the perception of MSU student use interaction hypothesis was not supported by males, it was partially supported by the females. The Frequent Binge Drinkers versus Nonbinging Drinkers comparison had a significant Interaction Model 06(5) = 14.01, p = .016, RZCM 5,... = .10, 82%“, = .13) and had a significant block improvement over the Additive Model (380) = 6.16, p = .013). The differentiation X 70 perception of MSU student use interaction term was the only significant individual predictor (see Table 6 and Figure 6, p. 73 for interaction effect). Students with a one unit higher interaction value were .975 (95% CI = .955, .996) times more likely to be a Frequent Binge Drinker than a Nonbinging Drinker. Students who had a difference in the interaction term equivalent to one standard deviation (21.57) were .58 times more likely to be a Frequent Binge Drinker than a Nonbinging Drinker. Table 6 Logistic Regression Predicting Female Students that are Frequent Bingers versus Nonbingers —- Perceptions of MSU Student Use Interaction Model b SE Wald OR 95% CI DSI-R -0.24 0.34 0.48 0.790 0.403 1 .546 BMI -O. 12 0.07 3.38 0.884 0.776 1.008 Age 0.09 0.15 0.35 1.096 0.810 1.482 MSU Use 0.01 0.01 2.03 1.008 0.997 1.019 DSI-R X MSU Use -0.03 0.01 5.48‘I 0.975 0.955 0.996 * p < .05, two-tailed. " p < .01, two-tailed. Similarly, the COA status interaction was not supported for males, but was partially supported for females. The overall Interaction Model for Frequent versus Occasional Binge Drinkers was significant (12(5) = 11.19, p = .048, Rza,“ 5n,“ = .07, R3natueumke = .10). The only significant predictor was the interaction between differentiation and COA status. Students with a one unit higher interaction value were 6.773 (95% CI = 1.209, 37.949) times more likely to be a Frequent Binge Drinker than an Occasional Binge Drinker (see Table 7, p. 72, and Figure 7, p. 74, for interaction effect). 71 Table 7 Logistic Regression Predicting Female Students that are Frequent Bingers versus Occasional Bingers — COA Status [ b 1 SE 1 Wald OR 95%01 Interaction Model DSI-R -0.27 0.34 0.61 0.767 0.395 1.489 BMI -0.12 0.06 3.75 0.887 0.786 1.001 Age -0.08 0.15 0.28 0.926 0.697 1.231 COA 0.35 0.46 0.58 1.413 0.579 3.447 DSI-R x COA 1.91 0.88 473* 6.773 1.209 37.949 * p < .05, two-tailed. *"‘ p < .01, two—tailed. The Comprehensive Model All of the variables of interest were entered simultaneously into a logistic regression in order to identify significant contributions of individual variables while controlling the influence of the remaining variables. Males. The Occasional Binge Drinkers versus Nonbinging Drinkers failed to reach a level of significance when all of the predictor variables were entered into one model (.603) = 19.27, p = .115). The overall models were significant for the Binge Drinkers versus Nonbinging Drinkers (36(13) = 41.1 1, p < .001, 8%,, r 3,... = .20, RZNM“. = .34), Frequent Binge Drinkers versus Nonbinging Drinkers (3803) = 64.24, p < .001, R20... 3. s..." = .40, Rsz = .60), and Frequent Binge Drinkers versus Occasional Binge Drinkers (.603) = 46.1 1. p < .001, RIC... 5,... = .26, 82m...“ = .35) (see Table 8, p. 78). Perceptions of best friend use (OR = 1.019) was the only individual variable to significantly improve the odds of being classified as a Binge Drinker versus a 72 0.8 ° 9 g 0.7 —W—‘—~»W—— ———— ____ we — E 045 E 0.6 —# §§05 ° — iu-ou-MSUUseHigh . E: 0.4 / “ 1-+-MSUUseAverage; 2 .5 03 -kww I—u—MSU Use Low “"8 - 1 t _ __- rrrrrr ég 0.2 F- ‘g 0.1 a n. O 1 r —~r Low Awrage High Differentiation Figure 6. The differentiation and perception of MSU student alcohol use interaction for female Frequent Binge Drinkers versus Nonbinging Drinkers. Note: Predicted probability scores > .50 indicate a greater probability for being a Frequent Drinker and < .50 indicate a greater probability for being a Nonbinging Drinker. Values are when controlling for BMI and age. 73 9.0.0 mum l I l i I if, I '1 il ('0 1 Ii 1. ‘l A ' + Non-COAS‘ p 00 i i l i i i i l pp AN Predicted Probability of Binge Drinking Category 0 O A or ii O a Low Average High Differentiation Figure 7. The differentiation and COA status interaction for female Frequent versus Occasional Binge Drinkers. Note: Predicted probability scores > .50 indicate a greater probability for being a Frequent Drinker and < .50 indicate a greater probability for being an Occasional Binge Drinker. Values are when controlling for BMI and age. 74 Nonbinging Drinker. Age (OR = 2.431) and perceptions of best fiiend use (OR = 1.040) were the only individual variables to significantly improve the odds of being classified as a Frequent Binge Drinker versus a Nonbinging Drinker. Age (OR = 2.43), perceptions of best friend use (OR = 1.018), and the differentiation X expectations of tension reduction (OR = .826, see Figure 8, p. 77, for interaction) were the only individual variables to significantly improve the odds of being classified as a Frequent Binge Drinker versus an Occasional Binge Drinker. Females. The Binge Drinkers versus Nonbinging Drinkers (3603) = 85.81, p < .001, R20... 8 3...... = .33, Rznmm = .47) , Occasional Binge Drinkers versus Nonbinging Drinkers (1203) = 45.05, p < .001, R20... 8; 5...... = .27, 182.....3...r....1,.3 = .36), Frequent Binge Drinkers versus Nonbinging Drinkers (x203) = 100.54, p < .001, R20,” 3..." = .52, 1?sz = .69), and Frequent Binge Drinkers versus Occasional Binge Drinkers (3803) = 39.46, p < .001, RIC,“ s..." = .24, Rszmag. = .32) comparisons were each significant as overall models (see Table 9, p. 79). Perceptions of best fiiend use (OR = 1.03 7), expectations of tension reduction (OR = 1.166), and the differentiation X expectations of tension reduction interaction (OR = .862, see Figure 9, p. 80, for interaction) were the only individual variables to significantly improve the odds of being classified as a Binge Drinker versus a Nonbinging Drinker. Perceptions of best friend use (OR = 1.030), expectations of tension reduction (OR = 1.147), and the differentiation X expectations of tension reduction (OR = .861, see Figure 10, p. 81, for interaction) significantly improved the odds of being classified as an Occasional Binge Drinker versus a Nonbinging Drinker. Perceptions of best fi'iend use (OR = 1.050) and expectations of tension reduction (OR = 1.251) were the only individual variables to significantly improve the 75 odds of being classified as a Frequent Binge Drinker versus a Nonbinging Drinker. Perceptions of best friend use (OR = 1.019) and expectations of tension reduction (OR = 1.081) were the only individual variables to significantly improve the odds of being classified as a Moderate Drinker versus a Low Drinker. Overall, few hypotheses received support from the disparate analyses. As an individual predictor, differentiation only changed the odds of group membership in the male Drinkers versus Abstainers comparison (see Table 10, p. 82). The differentiation X MSU perceptions interaction for the female high versus low drinker comparison indicated that differentiation moderated the main effect of MSU perceptions, which was contrary to the proposed hypothesis (see Table 10). Additionally, although there were significant interactions for male High versus Moderate drinkers, female frequent Binge Drinkers versus Nonbinging Drinkers, and female frequent versus Occasional Binge Drinkers, in each instance there was no significant main effect in either the differentiation variable or the other respective interaction variable (see Table 10). Chapter Five provides a more thorough discussion of the key findings. 76 8 0.9 i t W _.___,_L_ ‘5 08 ‘ m e —.:‘—.‘—“'-rr—r—’———"-—~~— —~~— '5 8 0.7 “ ~~~~ “5:4 Wm ’33 0.6 W ‘%r —W =men-TR High g ‘2 0,5 //W.W/—____°Q___ i-1--TRA\eragei g; as W—W W—W ___, _._ ”.5 L... 3 .g 0.3 WWWWW W WW 3°02 ___._..w------ E 0.1 WWWW W __ n. 0 “*‘f-"“"—‘ C“. T7 Low Average High Differentiation Figure 8. The differentiation and expectations of tension reduction interaction for male Frequent versus Occasional Binge Drinkers. Note: TR = Tension reduction. Predicted probability scores > .50 indicate a greater probability for being a Frequent Drinker and < .50 indicate a greater probability for being an Occasional Binge Drinker. Values are when controlling for all other variables in the Comprehensive Model. 77 H020 w 008308380 788; 84 78.0 AwA:m0 01:55 00.08100 AwA:m0 61:88 88:0 2::AOA:mA:m 33:03 AwA:m0 9.888 58:0 33:03 wA:m0 ©1288 88:0 . 78:38 8 000883: E: 0 :38; germ; 0 9588 m €95 OAw €08 OAw $85 CAN DE.” A .Nm N. A uA 08m A .03 A .3 chum szAA 92 A .23 obA o.ooo A .Nm obwo 1.9mm 9.3 A .NoA Sum: NAu A Aka... A .AVN 736 380982.. Abe AbA A 93 Abcm 0.”: Abcw UwArAw X ASmC $080388 93 Abom 9cm Abou who 0.3m w0mA 3.88 9qu AbAo A Aha... AbAo Auuc: AbA w DmAAw X w0mAm10RA obm Abou AQA AbNA VAN Ab; 00> Nuo 9qu NuA obAm and 93: DE.” X 00> obo Abwm Ab.» Abuo PS Amum H088: Away—28: A .3 A boa PA: A be» Now A .ouo UmAAw X H038: ”0898: 0.3 Abao 98 930 A80... 93: mooAuA E81088: Abe Aqu PS A.A AA Pun ob; UmAAw X 808A 5810030: 93 o.ooo PaA A.AcN Nam A.Amw .._ A bu. £98.88. 3.. h A .3. 38-88:. 78 H020 0 0033030880 78108 80 00:88 A0800 013080 0.3000100 9:00 01188 <088 2088080 00088:». A0800 01388 <088 300003 A0800 01188 <088 080:2: A0800 01188 <088 . 2088080 2088080 00088:»: A0800 95.8 9882 gas; gas; €08 0A~ €08 0A~ €08 0A~ €08 0% 00A-” 0.00 0.000 0.00 0.000 0.0m 0.000 A .3 A .30 0?: A .00 0.000 0.00 0.000 0.3 0.0 A 0 0.00 0.0.8 >00 A .0 A A .000 A .3 A bum 0.00 0.0a 0.2 0.00m :00 0080088 0.00 A000 0.00 A.00m 0.00 0.00.0 0.3 0.000 UmAAw X 300 0080088 0.00 0.00m 0.00 0.02 0.00 0.000 080 0.000 A000: 010:1 3.3.1. A .000 0.00: A .000 A9001. A .000 A931. A .0A 0 00A-” X A000: 010:1 :0 0.000 0.3 0.08 0.00 0.000 92 A000 00> 0.00 0.000 A00 0.00. 0.00 0.03 0.00 0.08 00:» X 00> 0.00 0.000 0.3 0.03 A.N0 0.000 0.00 0.00m H088: ”0808: A0801. :8 3.003.. :3 A000: A.NmA “TAN... A.00A 09-x X H088: ”01:08: 0.00... 0.000 A.A A... 0.03 :0 0.000 0.00 0.08 000:: 081008: 0. A m 0.000 0.3 0.30 0.00 0.00.0 A . A 0 A .000 003w X 000101081008: 0.00 A000 0.00 A08 0.00 A100 0.3 A000 .. m A .09 35-8181. 3.. .u A .0A. 23-81.00. 79 Predicted Probability of Binge Drinking Category Low Average High Differentiation Figure 9. The differentiation and expectations of tension reduction interaction for female Binge Drinkers versus Nonbinging Drinkers. Note: TR = Tension reduction. Predicted probability scores > .50 indicate a greater probability for being a Binge Drinker and < .50 indicate a greater probability for being a Nonbinging Drinker. Values are when controlling for all other variables in the Comprehensive Model. 80 0 on c. E '5 0.6 ._-________,*_n-_,____‘ _ f ' Z 3 <3, 0'5 / +TRAveragef 2g 0.4 ~—--———1_._TRLow ‘ l C 03 ./ _ ,-._m-__, ,_ ‘ L. , _.H_H_,_._,__- _ E3 0.2 ——-—«--—i+ - Wu- mm.___ _ r E 0.1%.”- _ m - u_ _H--_.-a ,_.__ 0 . _ Low Average High Dlflorentiatlon Figure 10. The differentiation and expectations of tension reduction interaction for female Occasional Binge Drinkers versus Nonbinging Drinkers. Note: TR = Tension reduction. Predicted probability scores > .50 indicate a greater probability for being an Occasional Binge Drinker and < .50 indicate a greater probability for being a Nonbinging Drinker. Values are when controlling for all other variables in the Comprehensive Model. 1.01.0 _o 9::an 01. ":0 mmmamga 1:11:15. $011082; $000502? ”0:201 8 9320:1010: 1.2 021 _Sw—om moan—om D1282; 2.01220 21m: <22; Em: 01:18.; 7101220 Em: <22; Em: <22; <22; res. r0«< <22; <22; <22; r0«< r05 <22; 202052; 01:18.; 01:18; 301230 2:23:02; U118; 01:18.; 7101220 01::2; 010:2; ww;10 71012 cum—aw "0:: 0:5 - - 511115: 583010: 2010—; Um?” X _Smc $200013; . n . Um?» x 00> Um?” x w0;:..10:1 Um?” x ._.0:;10: 131510: _‘ 02-x x m0010_r=:10010: w. 0008103. 30000:" 33:03 . 00820:». 3.09.03 3.09.03 _:m2; 9: 2; E: 2; <22; w_:m2; mm: 2; E: 2; <22; <22; m m . <22; m m . 20::Em1:m <2.2;. <2.2;. 003.30. 20::1:m1:m <2.2;. <2.2;. 00320:». U132; 20:05m5m 20:0_:m_:m w.:m2; 01:.»2; 20:0_:m_:m 20:0_:m_:m w_:m2; 95:2; 95:2; 95:2; 01:18.; mm;10 71010. 02-1223 0:. V 51111:». 588010: Z0101; Um?» x ch 1.2000103 - u 0;; x 00> + . Um?” x $210131 Um?” X 10:10: ”011010: Um?” x M0012 r5183: + ;1m:1100:1< 520801 ":0 011; 0m :01:m 1: ”:0 :1m:2 11:11:m 030m0Q B h A .o; 3.5-8103 - - 2211003; 1020201 ":0 011; 0». :01:m 1: ":0 :1m:2 11:11:m 020m0Q 2 u A .2 33.01.03 - ;1m:1100:1v. 1020801 ":0 011; 0». Sim 1: ":0 :1m:2 125—”Em Samoa. 2 n A .8 35.21.08 0 n >=:0=m: 50 1320010: $01020 «<8 amino»? 520 «<20 :0 301: 014.00; 01.0192 <210:_0. 208” 4:0 1:131:01 2011082; 20 32x01 0:; «<:0: ”:0 $20 71011 «<0; ;1m:110~:~ m; 0.: 0<0B= 3010. 2 <<:0: ":0 1303010: 3012 1:021:01 m 200: 13 S<030:3<2~m:1: ":0 011110: 01. 01111031 5.10202 82 Chapter Five This chapter provides a discussion of the results and an interpretation of the interaction effects, as well as a discussion on the limitations and implications of this study. Discussion of Results This study sought to understand the role of differentiation on college student alcohol consumption in order to address a lack of empirically validated studies regarding the validity of the tenets of Bowen theory. Additionally, this project’s incorporation of differentiation with one’s family of origin as a predictor in understanding individual decision-making processes had not been previously applied to the understanding of alcohol consumption. A series of logistic regression analyses were conducted to investigate the relationship of differentiation with general alcohol use (GAU) and binge drinking. Additionally, five variables that have been linked to college student alcohol consumption (perceptions of peer use, perception of best friend use, COA status, expectations of alcohol as a tension reducer or as a social lubricant) were examined for potential moderating effects on the relationship between differentiation and alcohol consumption. Overall the sample was comprised of a high number of students that drank alcohol and drank in high quantities. In a review of five national longitudinal studies, O’Malley and Johnston (2002) reported that in the 19903 around 85% of college students drank in the past year, approximately 70% drank in the past 30 days, and 40% had binge drinking experiences over the past two weeks. In this study, 89% of the subjects reported drinking in the past 30 days, which was not only higher than the longitudinal ranges for 30 day 83 use, but exceeded the levels of annual use. Additionally, the percentage of students that drank at binge levels (67% for the total sample, over 76% for males, and over 59% for females) exceeded the longitudinal studies’ ranges as well. Particularly striking was the fact that nearly half of the males (46.8%) drank at binge levels 3 or more times in the past two weeks. Comparatively, Hembroff and Kothari (2004) conducted a health assessment study of students at Michigan State University. A total of 88.8% of the 1329 students in the survey (six did not answer the question) reported consuming alcohol in the past 30 days. This study again points to the overwhelming use of alcohol by students attending Michigan State University. Additionally, students were asked how many times they had consumed five or more drinks at a sitting in the past two weeks. Forty seven percent of the respondents reported drinking at that level at least once in the previous two weeks. Although this figure is much closer to the 44% prevalence rates of national studies, it may still be an underestimate as females were screened with the five-drink criteria rather than the gender adjusted four-drink rate. However, the discrepancy between the two Michigan State populations may highlight that the students in this study were higher drinkers or more prone to report higher drinking than in previous studies. Dijfirentiation and Alcohol Consumption The predicted relationship between levels of differentiation and alcohol consumption received very little support. The only GAU comparison that had differentiation as a significant predictor of level of alcohol consumption was the male Abstainers versus Drinkers. The odds ratio supported the predicted effect of differentiation that students with lower levels of differentiation would be more likely to consume greater amounts of alcohol. This same relationship was found in the female Abstainer versus Drinker comparison, although the model was not significant. The findings indicate that, by itself as a risk/protective factor, differentiation only contributes in the decision to drink (or not) for males, and does not contribute to the likelihood that a student will be in one particular level of drinking category versus another. The differentiation hypothesis was not supported by any of the binge drinking comparisons. The results of the binge drinking analyses echo what was found in the GAU analyses, in that by itself differentiation contributes little as a risk or protective factor for how much alcohol students consume per drinking occasion. Four Factors of the DSI-R and Alcohol Consumption A closer examination of differentiation with alcohol consumption was implemented using the DSI-R’s subscales as separate predictor variables for each of the comparisons. Again, this model was very poorly supported by the analyses. The only . significant comparisons for the F our-F actor Model were in the two comparisons of extreme versus moderate drinking in males: High versus Moderate Drinkers and Frequent versus Occasional Binge Drinkers. Interestingly, taken as a full-scale, the DSI-R did not significantly predict either drinking category membership, yet the “1” Position subscales did predict both GAU and binge drinking categories, while Emotional Reactivity predicted GAU membership. Males with a greater sense of self, “1” Position, were less likely to be High Drinkers than those with a less developed sense of self. Similarly, males with a greater “I” position were less likely to be Frequent Binge Drinkers than Occasional Binge Drinkers. This finding indicates that individuals who have a more highly developed set of personal beliefs, values, and convictions are more likely to maintain a sense of moderation when they are drinking. These individuals with a more highly 85 developed sense of self drank five or more alcoholic beverages per drinking occasion once or twice in the previous two-week period. It can be interpreted from the GAU and binge drinking results that regardless of one’s level of “1” Position, one is no more likely to be a drinker or abstainer; however, when one does drink, he is more likely to do so in moderation in terms of monthly use (Moderate versus High Drinkers) and amount consumed per occasion (Occasional versus Frequent Binge Drinking). This suggests that maintaining a high sense of self could serve as a protective factor for consuming alcohol in more normative terms versus in extreme amounts. Surprisingly and counter intuitively, males who were more emotionally reactive were more likely to be Moderate versus High Drinkers. According to Bowen Theory, individuals who are more emotionally reactive are more likely to triangle something or someone in order to tolerate anxiety rather than deal with it directly. According to the results of this study, males who are more emotionally reactive are no more likely to be drinkers than not, but rather are more likely to stay within a parameter of moderate consumption than heavy usage. As the drinking categories were based on a Quantity X Frequency measure, it may still be true that students who were more emotionally reactive turned to alcohol more frequently but chose to drink less per drinking occasion. There appears to be a relationship supporting this notion as individuals who were more emotionally reactive were more likely to be Occasional versus Frequent Binge Drinkers; however, the odds of membership were not statistically significant in the model due to a large range in the odds ratio confidence interval (students were anywhere from .84 to 2.77 times likely to be a Frequent Binge Drinker). This suggests that emotionally reactive students may indeed drink more often, but at lesser amounts per drinking occasion. 86 Moderator Hypotheses Although there was very little support for the moderator hypotheses and alcohol consumption, some interesting interactions did occur. The only significant Interaction Models in the GAU analyses for males and females was for the interaction of differentiation with perception of MSU student alcohol use. For males, the interaction was significant for the High versus Moderate Drinker Comparison and for females it was in the High versus Low Drinker comparison. Taken by itself, as perceptions of MSU student consumption increased it was more likely that females were categorized as being High versus Low Drinkers. However, as the differentiation and MSU perception interaction term increased (high levels of each), it became less likely that the females were categorized as High Drinkers (Figure 4, p. 62). The effect of high or low perceptions of MSU student use only appears to be significant for females with low or average levels of differentiation. This indicates that as differentiation increases, there appears to be a protective factor against having high perceptions of use. Interestingly, as females with lower levels of perception achieve a higher level of differentiation, the likelihood they will be Low Drinkers diminishes, yet they retain an overall probability of remaining Low Drinkers. This suggests that as females become more highly difl‘erentiated they are more likely to drink at levels independent of what they feel is normative use. The nature of this interaction was different from what was hypothesized in that differentiation appears to moderate the relationship of perceptions of MSU student use. Similarly for the males, when perceptions of MSU student consumption were considered by themselves, it was more likely that students were High versus Moderate 87 Drinkers as their perceptions increased. This effect only appeared to be significant for males with lower levels of differentiation, although it remained minimally true with average levels of differentiation as they were 1.16 times more likely to be High Drinkers (Figure 3, p. 60). When males with high perceptions transitioned from average to higher levels of differentiation they actually become more likely to be Moderate Drinkers, indicating increased differentiation serves as a protective factor against high levels of alcohol consumption. Additionally, there was a trend for students with lower perceptions to be more likely to be High Drinkers as their levels of differentiation improved fiom average to high; although, this was a very minimal effect (they were only 1.05 times more likely when having a one standard deviation higher level of perception). Nevertheless, there is a unique relationship for males in that they are more vulnerable to be more extreme, rather than moderate, drinkers if they have higher perceptions of others’ use and low levels of differentiation. Moreover, as students obtain higher levels of differentiation there is a much lowered effect of their perceived level of student alcohol use. As was the case with females, the interaction was in the reverse order hypothesized in that differentiation appears to moderate the relationship of perceptions of MSU student use. These findings indicate that students with low levels (and moderate for females) of differentiation are more vulnerable to their conceived notions of how much alcohol the average student at MSU consumes. It is impossible to discern if the students create a reality of the MSU culture that fits their own established drinking behaviors or if their behavior matches what they feel is normative consumption. Nevertheless, having higher levels of differentiation appears to be a protective factor as there is a very minimal effect of perceptions in terms of students’ own drinking behaviors. While there were significant interactions for both males and females in terms of monthly alcohol use, there were only significant interactions for females in relation to binge drinking. The interaction of differentiation and perception of MSU student use had the same relationship for female Frequent Binge Drinkers versus Nonbinging Drinkers as it did for High versus Low Drinking females. Females with high perceptions were more likely to be Frequent Binge Drinkers when they had low levels of differentiation and actually reached a point where they were more likely to be Nonbinging Drinkers when they had high levels of differentiation (Figure 6, p. 73). Females with low levels of perception became slightly more likely to be Frequent Bingers when they had high levels of differentiation, although this effect was minimal in terms of predicting drinking categories. Again, this supports the notion that students with higher levels of difl‘erentiation drink independently of what they perceive to be normative use. Interestingly, the interaction did not fit the hypothesized relationship between the variables as neither was significant as an individual variable (according to the Wald statistic) and one variable did not significantly moderate the other. The other female binge drinking analysis that showed a significant interaction with differentiation was COA status in the Frequent versus Occasional Binge Drinking comparison. Similar to the previously described interaction, neither variable was significant by itselfin terms of predicting binge drinking categories. The non-COA females had a greater likelihood to be in the Occasional Binge Drinking category regardless of level of differentiation (Figure 7, p. 74). Female COAs were two-and-a-half times more likely to be Frequent versus Occasional Binge Drinkers when they had higher levels of differentiation. In terms of the theory of differentiation and triangling alcohol it 89 would be more congruent if COAs with higher levels of differentiation would binge drink less frequently as they had an ability to adopt different coping strategies than their alcoholic parents had. However, this hypothesized relationship purports that students are binge drinking in response to stress based on levels of differentiation rather than drinking for social reasons. Perhaps these students have normalized levels of acceptable use modeled by their parents and are not drinking as a means to alleviate anxiety. As there was no COA interaction for monthly use, another explanation for why COAs with higher differentiation may be more likely to be more extreme bingers is due to a perceived freedom fi'om their families-of-origin and are more apt to drink at higher levels (as modeled by an alcoholic parent) as they have afforded themselves greater fieedom to consume more per drinking occasion. As previously described, there has traditionally been a shortage of ecological studies regarding college student alcohol consumption. In order to test the effects of multiple variables and the context of varying degrees of differentiation all of the logistic regression equations were analyzed utilizing all of the variables of interest. As would be expected with the number of variables included in the analysis, all of the comparisons were significant with the exception of the male Occasional Binge Drinkers versus Nonbinging Drinkers. The most commonly occurring variable across the models was that students with higher levels of perception regarding their best fiiend at MSU’s alcohol consumption were more likely to be in the higher drinking category for the following: Moderate to Low Drinkers, High to Low Drinkers, Higher to Moderate Drinkers (males only), and in all of the binge drinking comparisons. When controlling for all of the other variables, differentiation failed to be a significant predictor of drinking category when considered independently. However, there was an interaction effect for perceptions of best fiiend usage (High versus Low Drinkers - females) and for tension reduction (Frequent versus Occasional BingeDrinkers - males; Binge Drinkers and Nonbinging Drinkers - females; Occasional versus Nonbinging Drinkers - females). Interestingly, the previously described interactions of perceptions of MSU student use and COA status were no longer significant when controlling for the other variables in the Comprehensive Model. There was an interesting interaction between differentiation and perceptions of best fiiend at MSU’s use as females had extreme probabilities of being a High or Low Drinker if they had low levels of differentiation (see Figure 5, p. 66). Females with high or average perceptions are almost always a High Drinker and those with low perceptions are almost always Low drinkers. As differentiation levels increase, students with average perceptions are only slightly more likely to be High Drinkers, while females with high perceptions are likely to remain High Drinkers and those with low perceptions are likely to remain Low Drinkers. The main effect of having highor low perceptions is minimally altered based on degree of differentiation, while increasing levels of differentiation appears to serve as a protective factor for females with moderated levels of perception as they are almost as likely to be Low Drinkers as High Drinkers. As students with low levels of difl'erentiation are extremely likely to either be a High or Low Drinker based on their perceptions of what their best friend drinks, it appears that these students have an inability to drink at differing levels than their immediate peer group. The probability of being in a particular drinking category became slightly less certain as levels of differentiation increased, regardless of level of perception. 9l Females with high and average expectations of alcohol as a tension reducer were always more likely to be a Binge Drinker versus Nonbinging Drinker (Figure 9, p. 80) or an Occasional Binge Drinker versus a Nonbinging Drinker (Figure 10, p. 81), regardless of their level of differentiation. This makes sense in a tension reduction fiamework as individuals would be more likely to drink at higher levels when they believed alcohol would help alleviate the anxiety. Although this study did not directly measure current levels of anxiety, the ability to tolerate anxiety was measured via differentiation. As students with high or average expectations exhibited increased abilities to tolerate anxiety increased (higher levels of differentiation) they were slightly less likely to be Binge Drinkers versus Nonbinging Drinkers (although minimal) or Occasional Binge Drinkers versus Nonbinging Drinkers, indicating a lesser prevalence of relying on binge drinking as a means cope with anxiety. Contrary to the tenets of differentiation, females with low tension reduction expectations in both comparison groups had an increased prevalence of binge drinking as their levels of differentiation increased; however, they consistently remained less likely than those with moderate to high expectations. One explanation of this relationship is that these students are merely drinking for the sake of drinking, rather than as a reaction to anxiety tolerance. As these students have low expectations that alcohol actually reduces tension, it is unlikely that they would rely on it is a means to cope with anxiety. Therefore, the increased prevalence of binge drinking based on increased levels of differentiation may be more attributable to a greater sense of fieedom that they have afforded themselves to drink more beverages when they chose to drink. The identical trend of tension reduction just described for females was applicable to the males. Gender differences demonstrated that in more highly differentiated males, 92 those with low tension reduction expectations have a greater likelihood to be a Frequent versus Occasional Binge Drinker, while males with high expectations are almost as likely to be in either particular category (Figure 8, p. 77). For females, the relationship of differentiation and high expectations of tension reduction were based on the comparisons at the lower end of the spectrum of binge drinking behaviors. For males this interaction was significant only for the more problematic and moderate categorical comparison. These gender differences indicate that differentiation as a potential protective factor may work differently for males and females. For females, increased differentiation may serve as a protector from escalating into binge drinking behavior as a coping mechanism. For males, greater difl'erentiation may protect them from a gateway to frequent incidents of heavy drinking as a means of coping. Limitations One limitation of the study is related to self-reporting of alcohol use. As students knew the purpose of the study was to understand how variables were related to alcohol use, there may have been a tendency to misreport their level of alcohol consumption. However, there were no difi'erences between categories of GAU or binge drinking for those that took the survey online or the paper-and-pencil version for either males or females. This finding suggests that the likelihood to over-report might be marginal as those who did not have contact with the researcher reported similar levels of use as those who did have contact. Another limitation for this study is the limited generalizability of the results to other populations. First, the sample was predominately Caucasian, which restricts the understanding of how the relationship differentiation has with drinking applies to a more 93 diverse population. Additionally, this sample appeared to be comprised of more drinkers than have been found in other studies. As mentioned, 89% of the subjects reported drinking in the past 30 days, which was not only higher than the longitudinal ranges for 30 day use, but exceeded the levels of annual use. Additionally, the percentage of students that drank at binge levels (67% for the total sample, over 76% for males, and over 59% for females) exceeded the longitudinal studies’ ranges as well. This may suggest that the sample may not be representative of other university students across the nation. This may also indicate that trends for use are much higher than the past orthat MSU has a unique culture of high alcohol consumption. Additionally, the effects of difl'erentiation may have been masked with such a high drinking sample. Recommendations for Future Research Although there was not overwhelming evidence supporting the role differentiation plays in college student drinking, the relationships that were significant may warrant future research to better understand this dynamic. It appears that differentiation by itself does not play a substantial role in the varying levels of alcohol consumption and binge drinking behaviors. Findings indicated that differentiation had significant interactions with perceptions of peer use, best fiiend use, COA status, and expectations of tension reduction. These interactions indicate that differentiation does indeed have some implications for college student drinking. Future studies should further explore the interaction of gender differences with differentiation as a risk/protective factor for alcohol use as well as for other behavioral coping strategies. . Future work with differentiation and college students should also incorporate longitudinal designs. As mentioned in the review of literature, the college culture is 94 unique in that a majority of students who drink at higher levels will drink at lesser amounts after graduation. However, for others, drinking behaviors in college may be a gateway to a lifetime of misuse or abuse. It would seem that differentiation would be one stable factor that would contribute to maintaining drinking behaviors beyond college. A person’s level of differentiation is unlikely to change without concerted effort on the part of the individual. Therefore, if an individual has a low level of differentiation in college she is likely to have a low level after college. It would be intriguing to examine the contexts in which levels of differentiation would continue to be risk or protective factors for students while they are in college as well as after graduation. Overall there was very little effect of COA status across all of the GAU and binge drinking analyses. This finding suggests that COAs attending college may have a greater sense of resiliency than non-college attending COAs. Another reason for a lack of a significant efl‘ect of COA status may be due to just screening for parental alcoholism rather than the contextual variation of parental alcoholism. Perhaps differentiation acts differently as a risk/protective factor under different circumstances. For example, a male COA with low levels of differentiation might be at greater risk if he had an antisocial alcoholic father with a 15 year history of alcoholism than he would be if he had a high functioning alcoholic mother with a recent onset of alcoholism. Future research should include typology of parental alcoholism, gender of the alcoholic parent, a gender of alcoholic parent-gender of COA interaction, the comorbidity of other mental health illness in the alcoholic parent, the presence of abuse either witnessed by or perpetrated on the COA, approximate number of years the parent has been drinking at alcoholic levels, and if the parent is in remission or is an active alcoholic. Higher levels of differentiation 95 may serve as a protective factor with these dynamics just as low levels of differentiation may be an additional risk factor. Not only are more comprehensive studies with types of alcoholics warranted, future research should incorporate a more ethnically diverse sample to enhance understanding of the relationship between differentiation and alcohol consumption, as well as the concept of differentiation in general. The DSI-R was normed on a predominately Caucasian sample and the few psychometric changes implemented in this study were with a predominately Caucasian sample. Future research with differentiation needs to examine if different ethnic groups are influenced in the same way by the various theoretical tenets of differentiation. Different ethnic groups may react quite differently from one another in their struggle for togetherness and individuality, anxiety tolerance, and triangling behaviors. Although Bowen states that it is theoretically universal to all cultures, there needs to be empirical validation of this assumption. Perhaps the most surprising discovery in the Comprehensive Model was the change in significance of individual variables. When controlling for all of the other variables the interactions between differentiation and perceptions of MSU student use and COA status were no longer significant. Additionally, the interactions between differentiation and perception of best friend use and expectations of tension reduction emerged as being significant contributors to the prediction of drinking categories. Although evidence has been provided for differentiation serving as a risk/protective factor, further research needs to analyze contexts in which differentiation remains a significant factor. Clinical Implications Although the results minimally supported for the differentiation hypotheses, certain clinical implications can be understood. Many universities offer or require alcohol counseling for students who misuse alcohol while they are residents of the university dormitories. Although there is a time limited nature to these counseling sessions, implementing a family-of-origin component might prove to be beneficial. If the scope of addressing the relationships between students and their families-of-origin is outside of the time parameters or the clinical training of the counselor, referral to a campus-based family or psychology clinic or a community-based family therapist might best serve the short- and long-term needs of students. As indicated by the various interactions in the analyses, levels of differentiation have a unique effect on risk factors for college drinking. If students become more aware of how their relationships with their families-of- origin are continuing to effect how they respond to anxiety tolerance while they are at school, they have a greater likelihood to respond in more objective than reactive ways. Family therapists and psychologists trained in family systems can be excellent resources for maintaining the overall well-being of university students. The psychotherapy literature refers to dynamic change process as being comprised of first- and second order change. First order change refers to the alleviation of symptomatology. In the case of alcohol misuse, this would be evidenced in a change of drinking behaviors. Second order change deals with addressing the dynamics involved in individuals’ lives that illicit symptomatic behavior. A treatment modality that emphasizes second order change might be particularly beneficial for college students based on the unique combination of stressors that they face: adjusting to living away fiom their family-of- 97 origin for the first time, a disparate number of demands in order to obtain academic success, balancing financial struggle of limited resources and paying tuition, living with peers, and becoming acclimated to the university culture. Students will react to these pressures in a myriad of ways. It is crucial that college counselors be aware of deeper motivations for problematic behaviors, such as alcohol abuse. If the first order goal of reduction in alcohol abuse is met, some students may simply develop a new form of psychopathology as a result of family relationships and coping styles learned in family contexts. As previously mentioned, a parental dyad can diffuse the relational anxiety by triangling their child. Although this may result in long-term consequences for the child, it brings short-term stability to the family. A consequence of parents triangling the child might be a deflated sense of self. Although being away at college may be a cathartic healing experience for some, it may be an overwhelming period for others. By gaining an understanding of students’ contextual dynamics, therapists, psychologists, and counselors will be able to help them adjust to university life. Summary The significant main effects of perception of the average MSU student’s use and expectations of alcohol as a tension reducer indicate that universities have the power to take a proactive stance in providing education regarding misperceptions that students may have about alcohol. One mechanism for providing reliable information about alcohol and how much is typically consumed is through various first year orientation programs. Additionally, information can be disseminated to the residence halls at various times throughout the year through student affairs programming or by providing information through campus newspapers as a way to educate both on- and off-campus students. The 98 focus of any of these educational programs should be on clarifying misperceptions regarding peer use as well as providing suggestions of coping strategies outside of alcohol use. As alcohol consumption is a normative part of the college culture, the university has an obligation to provide appropriate and factual information to its students regarding the use and misuse of alcohol. It is clear from the high numbers of students consuming alcohol that this problem could plague university life. REFERENCES Allgood-Merten, B., Lewinsohn, P. M., & Hops, H. (1990). Sex differences and adolescent depression. Journal of A bnormal Psychology, 99, 55-63. Andrews, M. P., Bubolz, M. M., & Paolucci, B. (1980). An ecological approach to study of the family. Marriage & Family Review, 3, 2949. Anonymous. (1972). Toward the difl‘erentiation of a self in one's own family. In J. L. F ramo (Ed.), Family interaction: A dialogue between family researchers and family therapists (pp. 111-173). New York: Springer Publishing Company, Inc. Archambault, D. L. (1992). Adolescence: A physiological, cultural, and psychological no man's land. In G. W. Lawson & A. W. Lawson (Eds), Adolescent substance abuse: Etiology, treatment, and prevention (pp. 11-28). Gaithersburg, MA: Aspen Publishers. Baer, J. S. (2002). Student factors: Understanding individual variation in college drinking. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, Supplement No. 14, 40—53. Bartle-Haring, S., Rosen, K. H., & Stith, S. M. (2002). Emotional reactivity and psychological distress. Journal of Adolescent Research, I 7, 568-585. Bartlett, T. (2002). Freshmen pay, mentally and physically, as they adjust to life in college: New survey provides more data on what happens during that crucial first year. The Chronicle of Higher Education, February 1, A35. Becvar, D. S., & Becvar, R. J. (1993). Family therapy: A systemic integration (2nd ed.). Boston: Allyn and Bacon. Benton, S. L., Schmidt, J. L., Newton, F. B., Shin, K., Benton, S. A., & Newton, D. W. (2004). College student protective strategies and drinking consequences. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 65, 115-121. Billingham, R. E., Wilson, W., & Gross, W. C. (1999). Parental divorce and consequences of drinking among college students. College Student Journal, 33, 322-327. Bishop, J. B., Lacour, M. A. M., Nutt, N. J., Yamada, V. A., & Lee, J. Y. (2004). Reviewing a decade of change in the student culture. Journal of College Student Psychotherapy, 18(3), 3-30. mo Black, C., Bucky, S. F., & Wilde-Padilla, S. (1986). The interpersonal and emotional consequences of being an adult child of an alcoholic. The International Journal of the Addictions, 21, 213-231. Bladt, C. W. (2002). Towards a psychodynamic understanding of binge drinking behavior in first-semester college freshmen. Journal of College Student Development, 43, 775-791. Bowen, M. (1966). The use of family theory in clinical practice. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 7, 345-3 74. Bristor, M. W. (1995). Individuals and family systems in their environments (2nd ed.). Dubuque, IA: Kendall/Hunt. Bronfenbrenner, U. (1977). Toward an experimental ecology of human development. American Psychologist, 32, 513-531. Bronfenbrenner, U. (1999). Environments in developmental perspective: Theoretical and operational models. In S. L. Friedman & T. D. Wachs (Eds), Measuring environment across the life span: Emerging methods and concepts (pp. 3-28). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Bubolz, M. M., Eicher, J. B., & Sontag, M. S. (1979). The human ecosystem: A model. Journal of Home Economics, 71, 28-31. Bubolz, M. M., & Sontag, M. S. (1993). Human ecology theory. In P. G. Boss, W. J. Doherty, R. LaRossa, W. R. Schumm & S. K. Steinmetz (Eds), Sourcebook of family theories and methods: A contextual approach (pp. 419-450). New York: Plenum Press. Byrne, B. M. (2001). Structural equation modeling with Amos: Basic concepts, applications, and programming. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Camatta, C. D., & Nagoshi, C. T. (1995). Stress, depression, irrational beliefs, and alcohol use and problems in a college student sample. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 19, 142-146. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2004). BMI - body mass index: BMI for adults: Body mass index formula for adults. Retrieved December 20, 2004, from http://www.ch.gov/nccdphp/dnpa/bmi/bmi-adult-fonnula.htrn . Clair, D., & Genest, M. (1987). Variables associated with the adjustment of offspring of alcoholic fathers. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 48, 345-355. IOI Clair, D. J ., & Genest, M. (1992). The children of alcoholics screening test: Reliability and relationship to family environment, adjustment, and alcohol-related stressors of adolescent offspring of alcoholics. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 48, 414- 420. Clapp, J. D., & McDonnell, A. L. (2000). The relationship of perceptions of alcohol promotion and peer drinking norms to alcohol problems reported by college students. Journal of College Student Development, 41, 19-26. Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 155-159. Coll, K. M., Shott, D., & Morris, J. (1999). An assessment of drinking patterns and drinking problems among community college students: Implications for programming. Journal of College Student Development, 40, 98-100. Cooney, R. 8., & Nonnamaker, J. B. (1992). Alcohol behavior: Commuting versus resident students. Journal of College Student Development, 33, 395-402. Cotton, N. S. (1979). The familial incidence of alcoholism: A review. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 40(1), 89-116. Demers, A., Kairouz, S., Adlaf, E. M., Gliksman, L., Newton-Taylor, B., & Marchand, A. (2002). Multilevel analysis of situational drinking among Canadian undergraduates. Social Science & Medicine, 55, 415-424. Dinning, W. D., & Berk, L. A. (1989). The children of alcoholics screening test: Relationship to sex, family environment, and social adj ustrnent in adolescents. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 45, 335-339. Dreer, L. E., Ronan, G. F., Ronan, D. W., Dush, D. M., & Elliott, T. R. (2004). Binge drinking and college students: An investigation of social problem-solving abilities. Journal of College Student Development, 45, 303-315. Elieson, M. V., & Rubin, L. J. (2001). Differentiation of self and major depressive disorders: A test of Bowen theory among clinical, traditional, and intemet groups. Family Therapy, 28, 125-142. Ellis, D. A., Zucker, R. A., & Fitzgerald, H. E. (1997). The role of family influences in development and risk. Alcohol Health and Research World 21, 218-226. Engs, R. C. (1990). Fanrily background of alcohol abuse and its relationship to alcohol consumption among college students: An unexpected finding. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 51, 542-547. 102 Fitzgerald, H. E., Zucker, R. A., Mun, E.-Y., Puttler, L. 1., & Wong, M. M. (2002). Origins of addictive behavior: Structuring pathways to alcoholism during infancy and early childhood. In H. E. Fitzgerald, K. H. Karraker & T. Luster (Eds), Infant development: Ecological perspectives (pp. 221-252). New York: RoutledgeFalmer. Fitzgerald, H. E., Zucker, R. A., Puttler, L. 1., Caplan, H. M., & Mun, E.-Y. (2000). Alcohol abuse/dependence in women and girls: Aetiology, course and subtype variations. Alcoscope International, Review of Alcoholism Management, 3, 6- 10. Friedman, E. H. (1991). Bowen theory and therapy. In A. S. Gurman & D. P. Kniskem (Eds), Handbook of family therapy (Vol. 2, pp. 134-170). New York: Brunner/Mazel. Fromme, K., & Ruela, A. (1994). Mediators and moderators of young adults' drinking. Addiction, 89, 63-71. Garbarino, C., & Strange, C. (1993). College adjustment and family environments of students reporting parental alcohol problems. Journal of College Student Development, 34, 261-266. Gisske, L., & Adams, M. E. (1988). Alcohol use and drinking behaviors among college students. College Student Journal, 22, 352-357. Goluke, U., Landeen, R., & Meadows, D. (1983). A comprehensive theory of the pathogenesis of alcoholism. In B. Kissin & H. Begleiter (Eds), The pathogenesis of alcoholism: Psychosocial factors (pp. 605-675). New York: Plenum Press. Gotham, H. J ., Sher, K. J., & Wood, P. K. (2003). Alcohol involvement and developmental task completion during young adulthood. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 64, 32-42. Grant, B. F. (2000). Estimates of us children exposed to alcohol abuse and dependence in the family. American Journal of Public Health, 90, 112-115. Grant, B. F ., Harford, T. C., Dawson, D. A., Chou, P., Dufour, M., & Pickering, R. (1994). Prevalence of DSM-IV alcohol abuse and dependence: United states, 1992. Alcohol Health and Research World, I 8, 243-248. Griffore, R. J., & Phenice, L. A. (2001). The language of human ecology: A general systems perspective. Dubuque, IA: Kendall/Hunt. Haemmerlie, F. M., Steen, S. C., & Benedicto, J. A. (1994). Undergraduates' conflictual independence, adjustment, and alcohol use: The importance of the mother-student relationship. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 50, 644-650. 103 Harford, T. C., Wechsler, H., & Seibring, M. (2002). Attendance and alcohol use at parties and bars in college: A national survey of current drinkers. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 63, 726-733. Hembroff, L. A., & Kothari, S. H. (2004). MSU student health assessment: Spring 2004. Michigan State University, Olin Health Center and the American College Health Association by the Office for Survey Research. Hewes, R.L., & Janikowski, T. P. (1998). Parental alcoholism and codependency: A comparison of female children of alcoholics and nonalcoholics in two college-age groups. College Student Journal, 32, 140-147. Hopkins, w. G. (2002). A scale of magnitudes for effect statistics. In: A new view of statistics. Internet Society for Sport Science. Retrieved June 18, 2005, from http://www.sportsci.org/resource/stats/effectmag.html Howard, M. C. (1992). Adolescent substance abuse: A social learning theory perspective. In G. W. Lawson & A. W. Lawson (Eds), Adolescent substance abuse: Etiology, treatment, and prevention (pp. 29-40). Gaithersburg, MA: Aspen Publishers. Johnson, V., & Pandina, R J. (2000). Alcohol problems among a community sample: Longitudinal influences of stress, coping, and gender. Substance Use and Misuse, 35, 669-686. Johnston, L. D., O'Malley, P. M., Bachman, J. G., & Schulenberg, J. E. (2004). Monitoring the future national survey results on drug use, 1975-2003: Volume 11, college students and adults ages 19-45 (NIH publication no. 04-5508). Bethesda, MD: National Institute on Drug Abuse. Jones, D. H., Harel, Y., & Levinson, R. M. (1992). Living arrangements, knowledge of health risks, and stress as determinants of health-risk behavior among college students. Journal of American College Health, 41, 4348. Jones, J. P., & Kinnick, B. C. (1995). Adult children of alcoholics: Characteristics of students in a university setting. Journal of Alcohol and Drug Education, 40(2), 58-70. Jones, J. W. (1983). The children of alcoholics screening test: A validity study. Bulletin of the Society of Psychologists in Addictive Behaviors, 2, 155-163. Kaplowitz, S. A., & Campo, S. (2004). Drinking, alcohol policy, and attitudes toward a campus riot. Journal of College Student Development, 45, 501-515. Karwacki, S. B., & Bradley, J. R. (1996). Coping, drinking motives, goal attainment expectancies and family models in relation to alcohol use among college students. Journal of Drug Education, 26, 243-255. 104 Kelly, V. A., & Myers, J. E. (1996). Parental alcoholism and coping: A comparison of female children of alcoholics with female children of nonalcoholics. Journal of Counseling and Development, 74, 501-504. Kline, R. B. (1998). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling. New York: The Guildford Press. Knight, J. R., Wechsler, H., Kuo, M., Seibring, M., Weitzrnan, E. R., & Schuckit, M. A. (2002). Alcohol abuse and dependence among US. college students. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 63, 263-270. Korcuska, J. S., & Thombs, D. L. (2003). Gender role conflict and sex-specific drinking norms: Relationships to alcohol use in undergraduate women and men. Journal of College Student Development, 44, 204-216. Kushner, M. G., Sher, K. J., Wood, M. D., & Wood, P. K. (1994). Anxiety and drinking behavior: Moderating effects of tension-reduction alcohol outcome expectancies. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 18, 852-860. Landers, D., & Hollingdale, L. (1988). Working with children of alcoholics on a college campus: A rationale and strategies for success. In M. Rivinus (Ed.), Alcoholism, chemical dependency, and the college student (pp. 205-222). New York: The Haworth Press. Lange, J. E., & Voas, R. B. (2001). Defining binge drinking quantities through resulting blood alcohol concentrations. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 15, 310-316. Larirner, M. E., Lydum, A. R., Anderson, B. K., & Turner, A. P. (1999). Male and female recipients of unwanted sexual contact in a college student sample: Prevalence rates, alcohol use, and depression symptoms. Sex Roles, 40, 295-308. Larson, J. H., & Wilson, S. M. (1998). Family of origin influences on young adult career decision problems: A test of Bowenian theory. The American Journal of Family Therapx 26, 39-53. Lawson, G. W. (1992). lntergenerational alcoholism: The family connection. In G. W. Lawson & A. W. Lawson (Eds), Adolescent substance abuse: Etiology, treatment, and prevention (pp. 41-69). Gaithersburg, MA: Aspen Publishers. Lecci, L., MacLean, M. G., & Croteau, N. (2002). Personal goals as predictors of college student drinking motives, alcohol use and related problems. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 63, 620-630. ' Lerner, R. M. (1991). Changing organism-context relations as the basic process of development: A developmental contextual perspective. Developmental Psychology, 2 7, 27-32. 105 Lerner, R. M. (1992). Dialectics, developmental contextualism, and the further enhancement of theory about puberty and psychosocial development. Journal of Early Adolescence, 12, 366-388. Lerner, R. M. (1993). Early adolescence: Toward an agenda for the integration of research, policy, and intervention. In R. M. Lerner (Ed.), Early adolescence: Perspectives on research, policy, and intervention (pp. 1-13). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Lerner, R. M., & Galambos, N. L. (1998). Adolescent development: Challenges and opportunities for research, programs, and policies. Annual Review of Psychology, 49, 413-446. Lerner, R. M., & Lerner, J. V. (1987). Children in their contexts: A goodness-of-fit model. In B. Lancaster, J. Altrnann, A. S. Rossi & L. R Sherrod (Eds), Parenting across the life span: Biosocial dimensions (pp. 377-404). New York: Aldine de Gruyter. Lerner, R. M., & Lerner, J. V. (1989). Organismic and social-contextual bases of development: The sample case of adolescence. In W. Damon (Ed.), Child development today and tomorrow (pp. 69-85). San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass. Lo, C. C. (1995). Gender differences in collegiate alcohol use. The Journal of Drug Issues, 25, 817-836. Marlatt, G., Baer, J. S., Kivlahan, D. R., Dimeff, L. A., Larirner, M. E., Quigley, L. A., et al. (1998). Screening and brief intervention for high-risk college student drinkers: Results from a 2-year follow-up assessment. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 66, 604-615. McCormack, A. S. (1996). Drinking in stressful situations: College men under pressure (1990 and 1994). College Student Journal, 30, 65-68. McGoldrick, M., & Carter, B. (2001). Advances in coaching: Family therapy with one person. Journal of Marital and Family Therapy, 27, 281-300. Noel, N. E., & Cohen, D. J. (1997). Changes in substance use during times of stress: College students the week before exams. Journal of Drug Education, 27, 363- 372. . O'Malley, P. M., & Johnston, L. D. (2002). Epidemiology of alcohol and other drug use among American college students. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, Suppl] 4, 23-39. 106 O'Sullivan, C. M. (1992). Adolescents in alcoholic families. In G. W. Lawson & A. W. Lawson (Eds), Adolescent substance abuse: Etiology, treatment, and prevention (pp. 419-427). Gaithersburg, MA: Aspen Publishers. Perkins, H. W., & Berkowitz, A. D. (1986). Perceiving the community norms of alcohol use among students: Some research implications for campus alcohol education programming. The International Journal of the Addictions, 21, 961-976. Perkins, H. W., & Wechsler, H. (1996). Variation in perceived college drinking norms and its impact on alcohol abuse: A nationwide study. Journal of Drug Issues, 26, 961-976. Poon, E., Ellis, D. A., Fitzgerald, H. E., & Zucker, R. A. (2000). Intellectual, cognitive, and academic performance among sons of alcoholics during the early school years: Differences related to subtypes of familial alcoholism. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 24, 1020-1027. Presley, C. A., Meilrnan, P. W., & Lyerla, R (1994). Development of the core alcohol and drug survey: Initial findings and future directions. Journal of American College Health (double check), 42, 248-255. Protinsky, H., & Gilkey, J. K. (1996). An empirical investigation of the construct of personality authority in late adolescent women and their level of college adjustment. Adolescence, 31, 291-294. Pullen, L. M. (1994). The relationships among alcohol abuse in college students and selected psychological/demographic variables. Journal of Alcohol and Drug Education, 40, 36-50. Reese, F. L., Chassin, L., & Molina, B. S. G. (1994). Alcohol expectancies in early adolescents: Predicting drinking behavior from alcohol expectancies and parental alcoholism. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 55, 276-284. Rodney, H. (1995). A profile of collegiate black adult children of alcoholics. Journal of College Student Development, 36, 228-235. Sadava, S. W., & Pak, A. W. (1993). Stress-related problem drinking and alcohol problems: A longitudinal study and extension of Marlatt's model. Canadian Journal of Behavioral Science, 25 , 446-464. Sax, L. J. (1997). Health trends among college freshmen. Journal of American College Health, 45, 252-262. Schulenberg, J., Maggs, J. L., Long, S. W., Sher, K. J., Gotham, H. J., Baer, J. S., et al. (2001). The problem of college drinking: Insights from a developmental perspective. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 25 , 473-477. 107 Schulenberg, J. E., & Maggs, J. L. (2002). A developmental perspective on alcohol use and heavy drinking during adolescence and the transition to young adulthood. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, Supplement No. 14, 54-70. Sher, K. J ., Gershuny, B. 8., Peterson, L., & Raskin, G. (1997). The role of childhood stressors in the intergenerational transmission of alcohol use disorders. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 58, 414-427. Sher, K. J., Walitzer, K. S., Wood, P. K., & Brent, E. E. (1991). Characteristics of children of alcoholics: Putative risk factors, substance use and abuse, and psychopathology. Journal of A bnormal Psychology, 100, 427-448. Shorey, H. S., Snyder, C. R, Yang, X., & Lewin, M. R. (2003). The role of hope as a mediator in recollected parenting, adult attachment, and mental health. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 22, 685-715. Skowron, E. A., & F riedlander, M. L. (1998). The differentiation of self inventory: Development and initial validation. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 45 , 23 5- 246. Skowron, E. A., & Schmitt, T. A. (2003). Assessing interpersonal fusion: Reliability and validity of a new DSI fusion with others subscale. Journal of Marital and Family Therapy. 29(2), 209-222. Smith, P. D., Wells, D. B., & Abdul-Salaam, K. (1997). Assessing alcohol problems in student populations. In P. C. Rivers & E. R. Shore (Eds), Substance abuse on campus: A handbook for college and university personnel (pp. 165-187). Westport, CT: Greenwood Press. Stewart, S. H., Karp, J., Pihl, R. 0., & Peterson, R. A. (1997). Anxiety sensitivity and self-reported reasons for drug use. Journal of Substance Abuse, 9, 223-240. Titelman, P. (1998). Overview of the Bowen-theoretical—therapuetic system. In P. Titleman (Ed.), Clinical applications of Bowen family systems theory (pp. 7-49). New York: The Haworth Press. Titelman, P. (2003). Emotional cutoff in Bowen family systems theory: An overview. In P. Titelman (Ed.), Emotional cutofif' Bowen family systems theory perspectives (pp. 1-66). New York: The Haworth Clinical Practice Press. von Bertalanffy, L. (1980). General system theory: Foundations, development, applications (Revised ed.). New York: George Braziller. von Eye, A., & Schuster, C. (1998). Regression analysis for social sciences. New York: Academic Press. 108 Waite-O'Brien, N. (1992). Alcohol and drug abuse among female adolescents. In G. W. Lawson & A. W. Lawson (Eds), Adolescent substance abuse: Etiology, treatment, and prevention (pp. 367-3 79). Gaithersburg, MA: Aspen Publishers. Wechsler, H., Davenport, A., Dowdall, G., Moeykens, B., & Castillo, S. (1994). Health and behavioral consequences of binge drinking in college: A national survey of students at 140 campuses. JAMA: Journal of the American Medical Association, 272, 1672-1677. Wechsler, H., Dowdall, G., Davenport, A., & Rimm, E. B. (1995). A gender-specific measure of binge drinking among college students. American Journal of Public Health, 85, 982-985. Wechsler, H., Dowdall, G. W., Davenport, A., & Castillo, S. (1995). Correlates of college student binge drinking. American Journal of Public Health, 85, 921-926. Wechsler, H., Dowdall, G. W., Maenner, G., Gledhill-Hoyt, J ., & Lee, H. (1998). Changes in binge drinking and related problems among American college students between 1993 and 1997. Journal of American College Health, 4 7, 57-68. Wechsler, H., Isaac, N. E., Grodstein, P., & Sellers, D. E. (1994). Continuation and initiation of alcohol use from the first to the second year of college. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 55, 41-45. Wechsler, H., Lee, J. E., Kuo, M., & Lee, H. (2000). College binge drinking in the 1990s: A continuing problem: Results of the Harvard school of public health 1999 college alcohol study. Journal of American College Health, 48, 199-210. Wechsler, H., Lee, J. E., Kuo, M., Seibring, M., Nelson, T. F ., & Lee, H. (2002). Trends in college binge drinking during a period of increased prevention efforts. Findings from 4 Harvard school of public health college alcohol study surveys: 1993-2001. Journal of American College Health, 50, 203-217. Wechsler, H., Lee, J. E., Nelson, T. F., & Kuo, M. (2002). Underage college students' drinking behavior, access to alcohol, and the influence of deterrence policies: Findings from the Harvard school of public health college alcohol study. Journal of American College Health, 50, 223-236. Wetchler, J. L., & Piercy, F. P. (1996). Transgenerational family therapies. In F. P. Piercy, D. H. Sprenkle & J. L. Wetchler (Eds), Family therapy sourcebook (2nd ed., pp. 25-49). New York: The Guilford Press. Williams, J. G., & Kleinfelter, K. J. (1989). Perceived problem-solving skills and drinking patterns among college students. Psychological Reports, 65, 1235-1244. 109 Woltcrsdorf, M. A. (1997). Neuropsychological effects of substance abuse. In P. C. Rivers & E. R. Shore (Eds), Substance abuse on campus: A handbook for college and university personnel (pp. 23-48). Westport, CT: Greenwood Press. Yeatman, F. R., Bogart, C. J., Geer, F. A., & Sirridge, S. T. (1994). Children of alcoholics screening test: Internal consistency, factor structure, and relationship to measures of family environment. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 50, 931-936. Zucker, R. A. (1994). Pathways to alcohol problems and alcoholism: A developmental account of the evidence for multiple alcoholisms and for contextual contributions to risk. In R. A. Zucker, G. M. Boyd & J. Howard (Eds), The development of alcohol problems: Exploring the biopsychosocial matrix of risk (V 01. NIAAA Research Monograph 26, pp. 255-289). Rockville, MD: Department of Health and Human Services. Zucker, R A., Fitzgerald, H. E., & Moses, H. D. (1995). Emergence of alcohol problems and the several alcoholisms: A developmental perspective on etiologic theory and life course trajectory. In D. Cicchetti & D. J. Cohen (Eds), Developmental psychopathology: Vol. 2. Risk disorder, and adaptation (pp. 677-711). New York: John Wiley & Sons, Inc. Zucker, R. A., Fitzgerald, H. E., Refior, S. K., Puttler, L. I., Pallas, D. M., & Ellis, D. A. (2000). The clinical and social ecology of childhood for children of alcoholics: Description of a study and implications for a differentiated social policy. In H. E. Fitzgerald, B. M. Lester & B. S. Zuckerman (Eds), Children of addiction: Research, health, and policy issues. New York: Garland Press. Zucker, R. A., Reider, E. E., Ellis, D. A., & Fitzgerald, H. E. (1997). Alcohol involvement of the life span: A developmental perspective on etiology and course. In P. C. Rivers & E. R. Shore (Eds), Substance abuse on campus: A handbook for college and university personnel (pp. 3-22). Westport, CT: Greenwood Press. Zucker, R. A., & Wong, M. M. (in press). Prevention for children of alcoholics and other high risk groups. In M. Galanter (Ed.), Recent developments in alcoholism: Vol. I 7. Research on alcohol problems in adolescents and young adults. New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum. 110 Appendix A Differentiation of Self Inventory — Revised (DSI-R) Differentiation of Self Inventory - Revised (DSI-R) “These are questions concerning your thoughts and feelings about yourself and relationships with others. Please read each statement carefully and decide how much the statement is generally true of you on a 1 (not at all) to 6 (very) scale. If you believe that an item does not pertain to you (e.g., you are not currently married or in a committed relationship, or one or both of your parents are deceased), please answer the item according to your best guess about what your thoughts and feelings would be in that situation. Be sure to answer every item and try to be as honest and accurate as possible in your responses.” ‘ 1. People have remarked that I'm overly emotional. l 2 3 4 5 6 Not at all Very 2. I have difficulty expressing my feelings to people I care for. 1 2 3 4 5 6 Not at all Very 3. I often feel inhibited around my family. 1 2 3 4 5 6 Not at all Very 4. I tend to remain pretty calm even under stress. 1 2 3 4 5 6 Not at all Very 5. I usually need a lot of encouragement fi'om others when starting a big job or task. 1 2 3 4 5 6 Not at all Very 6. When someone close to me disappoints me, I withdraw from him/her for a time. 1 2 3 4 5 ' 6 Not at all Very 7. No matter what happens in my life, I know that I'll never lose my sense of who I am. 1 2 3 4 5 6 Not at all Very lll 10. 11 12. 13. 14. 15. 16. 17. I tend to distance myself when people get too close to me. 1 2 3 4 S 6 Not at all Very I want to live up to my parents' expectations of me. 1 2 3 4 5 6 Not at all Very I wish that I weren't so emotional. l 2 3 4 5 6 Not at all Very . I usually do not change my behavior simply to please another person. 1 2 3 4 5 6 Not at all Very My spouse/partner could not tolerate it if I were to express to him/her my true feelings about some things. 1 2 3 4 5 6 Not at all Very When my spouse/partner criticizes me, it bothers me for days. 1 2 3 4 5 6 Not at all Very At times my feelings get the best of me and I have trouble thinking clearly. I 2 3 4 5 6 Not at all Very When I am having an argument with someone, I can separate my thoughts about the issue fi'om my feelings about the person. 1 2 3 4 5 6 Not at all Very I'm often uncomfortable when people get too close to me. 1 2 3 4 5 6 Not at all Very I feel a need for approval fi'om virtually everyone in my life. 1 2 3 4 5 6 Not at all Very 112 18. At times I feel as if I'm riding an emotional roller-coaster. l 2 3 4 5 6 Not at all Very 19. There's no point in getting upset about things I cannot change. 1 2 3 4 5 6 Not at all Very 20. I'm concerned about losing my independence in intimate relationships. 1 2 3 4 5 6 Not at all Very 21. I'm overly sensitive to criticism. 1 2 3 4 5 6 Not at all Very 22. I try to live up to my parents' expectations. 1 2 3 4 5 6 Not at all Very 23. I'm fairly self-accepting. l 2 3 4 5 6 Not at all Very 24. I often feel that my spouse/partner wants too much from me. 1 2 3 4 ' 5 6 Not at all Very 25. I ofien agree with others just to appease them. 1 2 3 4 5 6 Not at all Very 26. IfI have had an argument with my spouse/partner, I tend to think about it all day. l 2 3 4 5 6 Not at all Very 27. I am able to say "no" to others even when I feel pressured by them. 1 2 3 4 5 6 Not at all Very 113 28. When one of my relationships becomes very intense, I feel the urge to run away from it. 1 2 3 4 5 6 Not at all Very 29. Arguments with my parent(s) or sibling(s) can still make me feel awful. I 2 3 4 5 6 Not at all Very 30. If someone is upset with me, I can't seem to let it go easily. 1 2 3 4 5 6 Not at all Very 31. I'm less concerned that others approve of me than I am in doing what I think is right. 1 _ 2 3 4 5 6 Not at all Very 32. I would never consider turning to any of my family members for emotional support. I 2 3 4 5 6 Not at all Very 33. I ofien feel unsure when others are not around to help me make a decision. 1 2 3 4 5 6 Not at all Very 34. I'm very sensitive to being hurt by others. 1 2 3 4 5 6 Not at all Very 35. My self-esteem really depends on how others think of me. 1 2 3 4 5 6 Not at all Very 36. When I'm with my spouse/partner, I often feel smothered. l 2 3 4 5 6 Not at all Very 37. When making decisions, I seldom worry about what others will think. 1 2 3 4 5 6 Not at all Very ll4 38. I ofien wonder about the kind of impression I create. 1 2 3 4 5 6 Not at all Very 39. When things go wrong, talking about them usually makes it worse. 1 2 3 4 5 6 Not at all Very 40. I feel things more intensely than others do. 1 2 3 4 5 6 Not at all Very 41. I usually do what I believe is right regardless of what others say. 1 2 3 4 5 6 Not at all Very 42. Our relationship might be better if my spouse/partner would give me the space I need. 1 2 3 4 5 6 Not at all Very 43. I tend to feel pretty stable under stress. 1 2 3 4 5 6 Not at all Very 44. Sometimes I feel sick after arguing with my spouse/partner. 1 2 3 4 5 6 Not at all Very 45. I feel it's important to hear my parents' opinions before making decisions. 1 2 3 4 5 6 Not at all Very 46. I worry about people close to me getting sick, hurt, or upset. l 2 3 4 5 6 Not at all Very 115 Appendix B Analysis of the DSI-R with College Students Description of the DSI-R Differentiation was measured by scores on the Differentiation of Self Inventory- Revised (DSl-R). Subscale and firll-scale scores from the DSI-R were obtained by averaging the six-point Likert responses for each of the respective scales afier reverse coding the appropriate items. F ull- and subscale scores reflect a range from 1 (low differentiation) to 6 (higher differentiation). Descriptive Statistics The means for the subscales ranged from 3.37 to 4.57 and the DSI-R full-scale mean was 3.89 (see Table 11, p. 117). Each of the full-scale-subscale correlations ranged from moderate (.66) to high (.86) and were significant at p < .001 (see Table 12, p. 117). Additionally, each of the subscale intercorrelations ranged from low (.29) to moderate (.65) and were all significant at p < .001. The full-scale and subscale measures each illustrated high internal consistency reliabilities based on Cronbach’s alpha values (ER ct = .86; [P a = .77; BC on = .81; F0 on = .69; DSI-R a = .90). Each of the subscales and full-scale showed significantly different scores for males and females, although the relative effect sizes indicate these differences may be relatively trivial. The Emotional Reactivity scores of females (3.46) were significantly lower than males (3.72) (t (445) = -3.11, p = .002) with a low effect size (d = .30, 9,, = 0.02) illustrating that only around 2% of the variance in ER scores can be explained by “6 Hwy—o _ _. 385m Ba Dina—Eon m2. :5 Hon: Um?” Ba mccmoaam .38— mmBEn Kama «35.3 38: m0 ”mama 38.: m0 ”mama 385 m0 ”850'. m” Pun o.oo :5 I o.oo 93...... 93 :5 I 93 93...... o.oo :m I o.oo 3 Pg 9: _.ua I 93 ALM: Paw New I 93 uba: 9% _.ua I 93 mo 93 PE Now I Foo 93...... 93 Now I abo 93...... 9.5 PS I 98 mo 93 9am _.mm I 93 who: 9% N: I Pow PM»: 93 _.mm I MS 62* who 93 NC I 93 93... 0.3 N?» I 93 Pg. ohm PS I 93 _._ h A .3. 398:8. .2. h A .2. 35-8%? .363 _N. mono—macaw m9. En H08. Umrw Ea madman—om m” :U m0 m0 m” .I. .I II I :u an: I II I mm but. but. .I II m0 but. am: he: I Um?” .3: mi...» .3... Que... 3% A .2:w 35-8=8 ll7 gender differences. “1” Position scores of females (3.96) were significantly lower than males (4.15) (t (445) = -2.9l,p = .004, d = .28, r2“, = 0.02). Emotional Cutoff scores of females (4.68) were significantly higher than males (4.43) (t (445) = 3.56, p < .001, d = .34, 3,, = 0.03). Fusion with Others scores of females (3.27) were significantly lower than males (3.49) (t (445) = -3.66, p < .001, d = .35, rzpb = 0.03). Finally, the total DSI-R scores of females (3.84) were significantly lower than males (3.95) (t (445) = -l .99, p = .047, d = .19, 3..., = 0.01) F our-F actor Model Confirmatory factor analyses were run separately for males and females as well as with both genders combined in order to gain a comprehensive understanding of the four- factor structure of the DSI-R with college students. The methods used in Skowron and Friedlander (1998) in the creation of the D81 were replicated for this analysis. Individual questions fiom the scale were clustered into meta-items for each of the four subscales (Emotional Reactivity = ER, “1” Position = IP, Emotional Cutofi‘ = EC, and Fusion with Others = F0), with each meta-item containing a total of 3 to 4 items. For example, the 11 items in the ER subscale yielded indicators ER), ER;, and ER3, each containing 3 to 4 items. A total of 12 indicators were created (3 per subscale). Although the items in Skowron and F riedlander (1998) were randomly assigned to the respective meta-item cluster, this study will use the identical clusters for ER, IP, and EC used in their study (ER; = DSI—R items 1, 14, 26, 38; ER; = DSI-R items 6, 18, 30, 40; ER; = DSI-R items 10, 21, 34; [Pr = DSI-R items 4, 15, 27, 41; [Pg = DSI-R items 7, 19, 31, 43; 1P3 = DSI-R items 11, 23, 35; EC) = DSI—R items 2, 12, 24, 36; EC; = DSI-R items 3, 16, 28, 39; EC; = DSI-R items 8, 20, 32, 42). As the items for the F 0 scale were revised in the DSI-R ll8 (Skowron & Schmitt, 2003) only three of the original questions remained. Although the wording for these items changed, their respective order in the inventory remained the same; therefore the item numbers were entered into the same meta-items from the original DSI with the three added questions (items 44, 45, 46) randomly assigned to the indicator groups (F01 = DSI items 5, 17, 29, 44; F 02 = DSI items 9, 22, 33, 45; F 03 = DSI items 13, 25, 37, 46). The intercorrelations for the 12 indicators used in the confirmatory factor analysis are presented in Table 13 (p. 121). Following the original protocol, each of the indicators was permitted to load freely on its respective factor and was constrained to 0 on the other factors. Each latent variable was scaled to the first indicator by fixing its value to 1.00. The fit indices used to evaluate the model were also replicated from the original study: A chi-square to degrees of freedom ratio (ledf< 2.0); goodness-of-fit index (GFI > .90); adjusted goodness-of-fit index (AGF I > .80); and the root mean squared of the residuals (RMR < .10). The four-factor model is illustrated in Figure 11 (p. 125).The model showed a good fit of maintaining a four-factor structure (x2(48), N = 447) = 177.71 , p < .001, xz/df= 3.70, on = .94, AGFI = .90, RMR = .04). Although the significant chi- square and chi-square-to—degrees of freedom ratio values indicated a poor fit, there has been debate about the problematic nature of these analyses as related to sample size and has resulted in the development of various other goodness-of-fit measures including the others in this analysis (Byrne, 2001; Shorey, Snyder, Yang, & Lewin, 2003). The four-factor model maintained a good fit for when females were analyzed separately (x2(48, N = 246) = 129.54, p < .001, ledf= 2.70, GFI = .92, AGFI = .87, RMR = .04); however, it fell below the specified fit criteria for males (x2(48, N = 201) = 214.40, p < 119 .001, fi/ = 4.47, on = .86, AGFI = .77, RMR = .10). The model with differentiation as a higher order latent factor (Figure 12, p. 126) indicated that the DSI-R was a single and multidimensional construct for the combined sample (x260. N = 447) = 189.22, p < .001, xz/df= 3.78, GFI = .93, AGFI = .90, RMR = .05) as well as separately for females (x260. N = 246) = 132.19, p < .001, xz/df= 2.64, GFI = .92, AGFI = .87, RMR = .04) but not for males (x260, N = 201) = 144.89, p < .001, xZ/df= 2.90, GFI = .89, AGFI = .83, RMR = .06). After analyzing the male data, one item from the Emotional Reactivity (#38), “1” Position (#11), and Emotional Cutoff (#32) subscales were omitted afier exploring their respective effects on their standard errors and covariance estimates. In addition to making the necessary changes to the confirmatory factor meta-item clusters (ERr, 1P3, EC3), item #15 was randomly chosen to move from IPr to IP; so that the later indicator would maintain a mean of three items. The means for the subscales ranged from 3.37 to 4.52 and the DSI-R full-scale mean remained 3.89 (see Table 14, p. 123). Each of the full-scale-subscale correlations ranged fi'om moderate (.68) to high (.86) and were all significant at p < .001 (see Table 15, p. 123). Additionally, each of the subscale intercorrelations ranged from low (.20) to moderate (.66) and were all significant at p < .001. The full-scale and subscale Cronbach’s alpha values were not altered with the deletion of the three items (ER a = .86; [P a = .77; EC 01 = .80; F0 on = .69; DSI—R a = .90). The intercorrelations for the revised 12 indicator used in the confirmatory factor analysis are presented in Table 16 (p. 124). 120 How—o S 09.8.30: 253x m2 :8 Um?” :2: 9888 m? m? m? :0. :6 .3 m9 m9 m0... v.9 _.1 ON m? I I I I I I I I I I I m? hm: I I I I I I I I I I m? .31. .31. I I I I I I I I I no. :31. but. .31. I I I I I I I I =c~ .31. .31. .31. .81. I I I I I I I :6 .31. .31.... .31. .31. .31. I I I I I I ma. No.1. Mme... .31. No.1. .31. .Nm1. I I I I I mO~ .81. .31. .31. .NN: No.1. .31. own... I I I I mOm .81. but. be: .31. No.1. .51. .31. 6N: I I I m0. our... .31. .81. .31. .31. .3: .31. .81. his... I I won .81. .81. .NA: .3...» .3 .3 .31. .8 Joe 001. I mom 3...... 31. .31. one... N01. wan... .31. No.1. NAME. 35...... .31. 208” m” n magma?— Womomfia.“ to n :1. weaned m0 0 mBonmosw— 0:83 mo n Fame: 2:: 099m. m? u Umrw seam _. 3. mm. mm“ m? H Um?” :35 a. 3. mo. no“ m? H Um?” :35 8. 8. war a: H Um?” :23 A. 3. Na. 3“ =u~ H Um?” :35 q. S. 3. Au“ now H Um?” :23 2. um. um“ m9 H Um?” :23 N. 5. NA. mm“ m0“ H Um?” :35 w. 3. Mm. we“ mom n Ume :35 x. no. um. AN“ m9 n Umzw :23 m. 3. no. .3“ m0» H Um?” :35 o. MN. mu. AM mom u Ume :23 3. Nu. uq. .3. ate A .8. 35-8%? 1.x» A .3. gorgfia. 121 As was the case with the original analysis, the subscales and full-scale showed significantly different scores for males and females with minimal effect sizes (see Table 14, p. 123). The Emotional Reactivity scores of females (3.54) were significantly lower than males (3.81) (t (445) = -312, p = .002) with a low effect size (d = .30, 12,, = 0.02) illustrating that only around 2% of the variance in ER scores can be explained by gender differences. “1” Position score of females (3.98) were significantly lower than males (4.19) (t (445) = -3.03, p = .003, d = 0.29, rzpb = 0.02). Emotional Cutoff scores of females (4.63) were significantly higher than males (4.39) (t (445) = 3.24, p = .001, d = 0.31, r2", = 0.02). Fusion with Others scores of females (3.27) were significantly lower than males (3 .49) (t (445) = -3.657, p < .001, d = 0.35, rzpb = 0.03). Finally, the total DSI- R scores of females (3.84) were significantly lower than males (3.96) (t (445) = -2.lO6, p = .036, d = 0.20, 12,, = 0.01). The revised four-factor model is illustrated in Figure 13 (p. 127).The model showed a good fit of maintaining a four-factor structure in general(x2(48, N = 447) = 171 .42,p < .001, ledf= 3.57, GFI = .94, AGFI = .90, RMR = .04) as well as separately for females (x2(48, N = 246) = 150.79, p < .001, ledf= 3.14, GFI = .91, AGFI = .85, RMR = .05) and males (12(48, N = 201) = 106.48, p < .001, ledf= 2.22, GFI = .91, AGFI = .86, RMR = .05). The revised model with differentiation as a higher order latent factor (Figure 14, p. 128) indicated that the DSI-R is a single and multidimensional construct for the combined sample (x260, N= 447) = 181.83, p < .001, xZ/df= 3.64, on = .94, AGFI = .90, RMR = .04) as well as separately for females (3860, N = 246) = 153.37, p < .001, xZ/df= 3.07, GFI = .91, AGFI = .85, RMR = .05) and males (38(50, N = 201) = 122.74, p < .001, ledf= 2.46, GFI = .90, AGFI = .85, RMR = .05). l22 .320 E :33 mag Oman—$58: mom :8 ”Emma .38— Umzw Ba madman—om Hog: mmBEa 3&8 maaaom 2.8: m0 ”mama K8: m0 ”mama Zoe: mD ”36ml mw o.oo 93 _.oo 1 o.oo 931. o.oo _.oo 1 o.oo 931. o.oo _.No 1 o.oo =u Aoq o.oo To 1 o.oo PS1. ohm ~._o I o.oo o.oo»... o.ow TE 1 o.oo mo 93 o.oo _.S 1 o.oo o.oo: o.oo _.2 I o.oo p.31. o.oo who I o.oo m0 o.oo o.on _.um I o.oo who... o.oo N: 1 o.oo. who: o.oo _.mm 1 o.oo Umzfl o.oo o.oo PM. 1 who o.oo... o.oo NAN I Mt o.oo. o.oo NB I who ... h A .om. go-§_ao. 1. h A .of 25.8%? Haw—o _m Ooflimmoam m9. :5 wn150 200 250 30 Day Quantity X Frequency Alcohol Consumptlon Figure 15. General alcohol use (Quantity X Frequency) of male drinkers for the Past 30 days. 148 8 1 Frequency N o l 0.00 30.00 60.00 90.00 120.00 150.00 Quantity X Frequency of Alcohol Use Figure 16. General alcohol use (Quantity X Frequency) of female drinkers for the Past 30 Days 149 30- Frequency 20- 0.0 2.0 4.0 > 6.0 8.0 10.0 12.0 14.0 The number of days in the past 2 weeks the subject drank at binge levels Figure 17. Frequency of male binge drinking over the past 2 weeks. 150 100— Frequency is O l 20- 00 2.0 4.0 6.0 8.0 10.0 12.0 The number of times the subject drank at binge levels Figure 18. Frequency of female binge drinking over the past 2 weeks. 151 Table 17 Frequency of Binge Drinking Categories by Gender Total Sample Males Females Frequency Percent Frequency Percent Frequencyl Percent Abstainers 49 1 1.0 16 8.0 33 13.4 “133523 98 2"9 31 15.4 67 27.2 $2;ng ‘35 302 60 29.9 75 30.5 Priming ‘65 36'9 94 46.8 71 28.9 GA U Conversion Whereas binge drinking categories were established in multiple studies, no clear norms for creating quantity frequency categories were present in the literature. It was decided that abstainers would naturally be one category and it needed to be determined if there would be enough of variation between the categories to create three additional variables (low-, moderate-, and high drinking categories by splitting the frequency distribution of those that consumed alcohol into thirds) or only two categories (low and high categories by a median split). As it was established from the test of association with binge drinking that males and females were to be analyzed separately (see Appendix K), frequency distribution points were utilized to create categories for males and females separately. A one-way AN OVA testing the 4 category model indicated that the moderate group did show differences from the low and high groups on comparisons to the DSl-R full- and subscale measures. Therefore, the positively skewed Quantity X Frequency variable was converted into a general alcohol use variable comprised of four categories (see Table 18, p. 153). 152 Haw—a ; manage! e». O>C Omnamomom 3. @232 H08— mmBEo Zw—om moan—om mama—egg $2.83 mane—nave $82: :35 m0 M.,—.2533 moans» Zoe: m0 sarcasm—09m so _ _ _ a m .. l mu _ w .A 2. .. _.ros so 2 3 3a 3.8 «a a 8.8 93 .30 9.5.83 38888 E 8 3 8.” 3: K.,: 8 was 8.3 o.oo 0:388 Em: . as we ac Nob Sun; the K we; 3.? 3.9» 6:388 153 Appendix K Individual Variable Descriptions Gender With the transformation of the skewed continuous data to categorical data, logistic regression was immemented to test the relationship between differentiation and the moderating variables with the outcomes of alcohol consumption. Prior to examining the main and interaction effects with logistic regression, males and females were tested for differences in alcohol consumption to avoid the interpretation of three-way interaction effects in the analyses. In order to test the hypothesis that gender and binge drinking behaviors were independent of each other (no relationship), a chi-square test of association was implemented with a .05 or level. The results indicated that gender and binge drinking were related 352(3) = 19.66, p < .001 and the Cramer’s phi (9c = .21) indicated that the association was relatively small. Since there was a significant association between gender and binge drinking, males and females were analyzed separately for all firrther analyses. Race Of the 447 subjects analyzed in this study, 373 identified themselves as Caucasian (83.4%), 45 as African American (10.1%), 5 as Hispanic/Latino (1.1%), 20 as Asian American (4.5%), l as Native American (.2%), and 3 as Other (.7%) (for gender comparisons see Table 19, p. 155). A chi-square test of association indicated that there was not a relationship between gender and race for this sample x26) = 7.214, p = .205. As this sample had such a limited numbers of non-Caucasians, this variable was excluded from all future regression analyses. l54 Table 19 Frequency of Race by Gender Total Sample Males Females Frequency Percent Frequency Percent Frequency Percent Caucasian 373 83.4 169 84.1 204 82.9 African American 45 10.1 17 8.5 28 11.4 H'SPmc/ 5 1.1 2 1.0 3 1.2 Latino Asian American 20 4.5 10 5.0 10 4.1 Native American 1 .2 0 0 1 .4 Other 3 .7 3 1.5 0 0 Age The age of participants for this study was restricted to subjects who were between 18 and 22, reflecting the typical age range of traditional students. Although males were significantly older (19.72, SD 1.24) than females (19.45, SD, 1.19) (t (445) = -2.39, p = .017, d = 0.22, 3,, = 0.01), the small effect size indicates this difference was relatively trivial. BMI Values of BMI are typically used to categorize weight by adjusting for height. The traditional weight ranges for adults are: Underweight (BMI < 18.5), Normal (18.5 - 24.9), Overweight (25.0 - 29.9), and Obese (30.0 _>_ BMI). The categories were not implemented in this study, as the BMI values were maintained as a continuous measure to serve as a proxy for alcohol tolerance based on weight. Males had a significantly 155 greater BMI value (24.62, SD = 3.78, range 17.72 to 41.38) than females (22.80, SD = 4.09, range 17.16 to 47.50) (t (445) = -4.85, p < .001, d = 0.46, 12,, = 0.05). COA COA status was determined by subjects’ responses to created gender-specific versions of the Children of Alcoholics Screening Test (CAST). Scores of six or more “yes” responses on the 29—item scales of either the F -CAST (for fathers) or M-CAST (for mothers) indicate the presence of an alcoholic parent. High internal consistency reliabilities based on Cronbach’s alpha values were found for both the M-CAST (males 01 = .96, females or é .96) and F-CAST (males 11 = .95, females or = .96). Almost 80% of males and females had parents who were not identified as being alcoholic (see Table 20). A chi-square test of association indicated that there was not a significant relationship between gender and the number of parental alcoholics (based on categories of O, 1, 2) for this sample (12(2) = 5.24, p = .073). Table 20 Frequency of Parental Alcoholism by Gender Males Females Frequency Percent Frequency Percent No Alcoholic Parents 169 84.1 195 79.3 One Alcoholic Parent 22 10.9 44 17.9 Two Alcoholic Parents 10 5.0 7 2.8 Due to the low fi'equency of one and two alcoholic parent families, tests of association were conducted to validate combining these two categories into one variable, creating a dichotomy of being a COA or not. There was no significant relationship between the 3 categories of parental alcoholism with frequency of binge drinking for 156 males (x2(6) = 10.31, p = .1 12) or females (12(6) = 6.17, p = .405, see Table 21 for gender distribution). Similarly, there was no a significant relationship between the 3 categories of parental alcoholism with general alcohol use for males (38(6) = 7.71, p = .260) or females (36(6) = 1.78, p = .939, see Table 22 for gender distribution). Table 21 Frequency of Parental Alcoholism and Binge Drinking by Gender . Nonbinging Occasional Frequent Abstainer Drinker Bmge Binge Drinker Drinker No alcoholic parents 16 27 51 75 Males 1 Alcoholic Parent 0 4 6 12 2 Alcoholic Parents 0 0 3 7 No alcoholic parents 24 56 56 59 Females 1 Alcoholic Parent 8 8 18 10 2 Alcoholic Parents 1 3 1 2 Table 22 Frequency of Parental Alcoholism and GAU by Gender . Low Moderate High “5'31““ Drinker Drinker Drinker No alcoholic parents 16 54 52 47 Males 1 Alcoholic Parent 0 7 7 8 2 Alcoholic Parents 0 2 3 5 No alcoholic parents 24 6O 51 6O Females 1 Alcoholic Parent 8 13 11 12 2 Alcoholic Parents 1 3 1 2 Individuals who had either one or two alcoholic parents were combined into one COA category. As was the case with the three categories of parental alcoholism, there was no significant relationship between gender and COA status 080) = 1.69, p = .193). 157 There was no significant relationship between the two categories of parental alcoholism with frequency of binge drinking for males (36(3) = 7.02, p = .071) or females (36(3) = 3.22, p e .359, see Table 23 for gender distribution). Although neither test was significant, the p values for the dichotomous COA variable were closer to significance than were the original three category variable. There also was no significant relationship between the two categories of parental alcoholism with general alcohol use for males (38(3) = 7.04, p = .071) or females (78(3) = 1.12, p = .772, see Table 24 for gender distribution). Since the COA variable slightly improved the frequency distribution and did not alter the relationship with alcohol use, the dichotomous variable was utilized for all future analyses. Table 23 Frequency of COA Status and Binge Drinking by Gender Occasional Frequent Abstainer “winging Binge 03:5: (3 Drmk' er Dnnk' er (1-2 or times/week) more/week) Males Not a COA 16 27 51 75 COA 0 4 9 19 Not a COA 24 56 56 59 Females COA 9 11 19 12 Table 24 Frequency of COA Status and GAU by Gender . . Moderate High Abstainer Low Drinker Drinker Drinker Males Not a COA 16 54 52 47 COA 0 9 10 13 Females Not a COA 24 60 51 60 COA 9 16 12 14 158 Expectation of Alcohol as a Tension Reducer Nine questions fiom Kushner et al.’s (1994) measure of alcohol outcome expectancies comprise the Tension Reduction subscale. The subjects’ responses on a 5- point scale were summed with higher scores reflecting greater tension reduction expectations. The Tension Reduction subscale had a high internal consistency reliability based on Cronbach’s alpha value for both males (01 = .91) and females (01 = .92). Males had higher expectations of alcohol as a tension reducer (15.07, SD = 8.73) than females (13.15, SD = 8.81) (t (445) = -2.39, p = .022, d = 0.22, r2» = 0.01) although the point- biserial correlation indicated that gender only accounts for about 1% of the variance in expectations of alcohol as a tension reducer. Expectation of Alcohol as a Social Lubrication Eight questions from Kushner et al.’s (1994) measure of alcohol outcome expectancies comprise the Social Lubrication subscale. The subjects’ responses on a 5- point scale were summed with higher scores reflecting greater social lubrication expectations. The Social Lubrication subscale had a high internal consistency reliability based on Cronbach’s alpha value for both males (01 = .89) and females (01 = .90). Although males had higher expectations of alcohol as a social lubricant (13.93, SD = 7.89) than females (10.97, SD = 7.74) (t (445) = -3.99, p < .001, d = 0.38, rzpt, = 0.03), only 3% of the variance of expectations of alcohol as a social lubricant can be accounted for by gender. Perceptions of A verage MSU Student 's Alcohol Use A total drinks-per-month variable was calculated by multiplying the number of days by the average number of drinks subjects believed the average Michigan State 159 student consumed in the past 30 days. Females (57.57 drinks-per—month, SD = 38.75) and males (50.89 drinks-per-month, SD = 37.22) did not significantly differ from one another in their perceptions of MSU student alcohol consumption (t (445) = 1.85, p = .065). Perceptions of Subjects ’ Best Friend at MS‘U ’s Alcohol Use A total drinks-per-month variable was calculated by multiplying the number of days by the average number of drinks subjects believed their best friend at Michigan State consumed in the past 30 days. Females had significantly lower perceptions of their best fiiends alcohol usage (36.88, SD = 41.48) than males (68.16, SD = 72.43) (t (303.78) = -5.44, p < .001, d = 0.54, 12,3, = 0.09), although only 9% of the variation in perceptions could be attributed to the subjects’ gender. Tests of Multicollinearity A summary description of means and standard deviations is provided in Table 25 (p. 160) After transforming the respective independent variables, correlation analyses were done separately for males (see Table 26, p. 162) and females (see Table 27, p. 163). Table 25 Frequency of Independent Variables by Gender Males Females Mean SD Range Mean SD Range Age 19.45"I 1.19 18-22 1972" 1.24 18—22 BMI 24.62" 3.78 17.72 - 41.38 22.80“ 4.09 17.16 - 47.5 Tension reduction 1508* 8.73 0 - 33 1315* 8.81 0 — 36 Social Lubrication 13.93“ 7.89 0 — 30 10.97" 7.74 0 — 30 MSU Student Use 50.89 37.22 6 — 176 57.58 38.75 4 — 180 Best Friend Use 68.16” 72.43 0 — 387 36.88" 41.48 0 — 180 * p < .05, two-tailed. ** p < .01, two-tailed. l60 Ignoring the correlations between the DSI-R full scale. with the 4 subscale measures (as the full- and subscales will not be analyzed in the same equations) the correlations ranged from .00 to .79 for males and .00 to .71 for females. As logistic regression does not have a measure directly equivalent to the R2 value of OLS regression, tests of multicollinearity could not be directly computed using the variance inflation factor formula (V IF) (von Eye & Schuster, 1998). In order for an exploratory analysis of multicollinearity to be conducted, liner regressions with the positively skewed continuous GAU and Binge Drinking variables were analyzed using the VIP. The VIP values were identical for GAU and Binge Drinking outcomes. None of the respective regression equations (differentiation only, five equations of differentiation with interaction terms, and the comprehensive equation including all variables) had VIF values reaching a value of 10.0 — a rule of thumb for multicollinearity (von Eye & Schuster, 1998) (see Table 28, p. 164, for VIP values for the Comprehensive Model which contained all of the variables). Therefore it was concluded that multicollinearity was not a concern for the remaining analyses. 161 mono—macaw 31330363 55628 1 3m? masooa mw wagon—o Amwv :0 wagon—o :3 m0 wagon—o Ame m0 mccmoao GHQ w7= >mo 00> Ema 3538 wow" Rona H088: non—Como: mega r5108: 0 .1. :53 n a: £62m. n 363m." w.” n 330303 cm com” $256 :8. a.” u oxeoosmoam cm Same: 898:0? 9.. n 9888225 0». 8&3 _cclomnoz. _._ A "Serge—ma. _._... A .o go-§_oa. 162 mono—mnonm men Engages» 556.8 I moan—o mcgmoa mw wagon—o :0 mccmoao . . m0 mccmofla m0 wagon—o get: 163 >mn 00> ch men—0:8 wow» Rosa H088: 398:0: moon: 5&3.er u 0 38m 0 a: mBEm. u madame mm. N 380303 em com» «lo—aw :mo. H” u 06008203 cm 838: 898:0? mr n 0633203 om moan. 3.61820? _._ A 25-828. .1. A.A: 25-859 Table 28 VIP for Independent Variables Males Females DSI-R 1.54 1.67 Age 1.11 1.05 Body Mass Index 1.09 1.09 Perceptions of MSU student use 1.21 1.12 Perceptions of best friend at MSU’s use 1.45 1.35 Expectations of alcohol as a tension reducer 2.97 2.17 Expectations of alcohol as a social lubricant 2.93 2.22 COA status 1.07 1.08 DSI-R X Perceptions of MSU student use 1.11 1.11 DSI-R X Perceptions of best fiiend at MSU’s use 1.52 1.44 DSI-R X ExEctations of alcohol as a tension reducer 3.47 2.79 DSI-R X Expectations of alcohol as a social lubricant 3.21 2.70 DSI-R X COA status 1.35 1.33 Note: All variables have been centered around the mean, with the exception of the dummy variable of COA status I64 Appendix L Analyses of General Alcohol Use As previously described, the continuous General Alcohol Use (GAU) measure (30 day Quantity X Frequency) was converted into a categorical variable consisting of 4 levels: Abstainers, Low Drinkers, Moderate Drinkers, and High Drinkers (see Appendix I). As the four categories reflect increasing degrees of alcohol consumption, the nature of this study’s originally stated hypotheses for a continuous dependent variable (e. g., students with lower levels of differentiation will be more likely to have greater amounts of general alcohol consumption) was maintained with the categorical analyses (e. g., students with lower levels of differentiation will be more likely to have a greater relationship with High Drinkers than Low Drinkers when controlling for BMI and age). As logistic regression is based on a dichotomous dependent variable, the four GAU categories were contrasted in ways such that four unique outcome dichotomies were created: 1. Drinkers (Low, Moderate, High) versus Abstainers 2. Moderate Drinkers versus Low Drinkers 3. High Drinkers versus Low Drinkers 4. High Drinkers versus Moderate Drinkers In each instance, the variable with the greater amount of alcohol consumption was coded as a “1” in SPSS 13.0 and the lesser category was coded as a “0.” For example, in the Drinkers versus Abstainers analyses, all persons in the Low, Moderate, and High Drinking categories were coded as a “l” and Abstainers were coded as a “0.” 165 Drinkers versus Abstainers Diflerentiation and GA U Males. The Basic Model was significant according to the model chi-square (x2(3) = 18.00, p < .001, Ric,“ 5,," = .09, PM...” = .20). The model illustrated a 92% success rate in predicting the correct drinking classifications, however, this is the same value if someone were to predict an individual was a drinker with every case (a model with no predictors), indicating the Basic Model adds nearly nothing to the ability to predict individuals that drink from those that abstain. When an individual is at an average BMI level and age, differentiation had a significant negative coeflicient indicating that the odds of drinking are multiplied by .190 (95% CI = .068, .530) with a one unit difference in differentiation, which is a 81% (1 - .19) decrease (see Table 29, p. 167). Additionally, when differentiation and age are at average levels, an individual is 2.24 times more likely to drink as they gain one year of age (e.g., 19- to 18-year olds, 20- to 19-year olds). Females. The Basic Model was not significant according to the model chi-square (£0) = 6.91, p = .075), indicating that there was no effect of the independent variables, taken together, on the dependent variable (see Table 29, p. 167). F our factors of the DSI-R (Emotional Reactivity, “I ” Position, Emotional C utofl and Fusion with Others) with GA U Males. As was the case in the Basic Model, the overall four factor model was significant (36(6) = 18.21, p = .006, R20», & s...“ = .09, 82mm. = .20). This model had the same successful prediction rate of 92% that the Basic Model and having no model flea—a No rommmno Womaommmo: wmnamnzsm 9338 an: 013w I Omanqoszmaoa Zea—aw maaaom a mm €65 OW 8.x. 9 e mm $35 or 8.x. 9 Um?” -_ .3 93 8.3.... 93o o.oam ouwo -o.$ 93 #8 a PM; 0.3x o.ouo 95 .92 o.ou Pow o.omc Pmfi : .3 .98 obs mud 993 0.30 foo. >ma of on: o.oo: wwwo fwww are: o.oo 93 ohm _.owm 93m _.hmo ate A bu. 23-8:09 rate A .2. 396:3. Haw—o we homage ”3388: 32:on mEaga 9m: 9.5x i mean 1888 :33 moan—om w mm (Sea OW 3.x. 9 e mm (5.5 OW 3.x. 9 wages”— Woeomia. .93 ohm _.ws chum PNNN T3» 93 ohm PI :8 993 _.08 :1 $830: -93 93 93 0.95 obmw _.uua -93 9.63 93 9me 933 ram mangoes. OER“ -o.~o 9.3 9; Pawn 930 News 93 ohm o.oo _.cow cud. _.qfi 358: SE. 0563 -obm PS cow oboe chow Puma ion; owe Pam 93¢ obB _.cmo 9,3 -93 0.3 98 923 Pug :3 -93 PE Nd 98.“ 33.6. _.Sw >mo Pmo out 93: NNNN _.Nuc are: o.oo 93 ohm _.oE chem _.Aom e m A .3. 236:8. 1g A .3. 298:8. 167 had. None of the four factors significantly changed the odds of group membership (see Table 30, p. 167). Females. As was the case in the Basic Model, the overall four-factor model was not significant (12(6) = 9.21,p = .162, see Table 30, p. 167). Dijferentiation & Perception of A verage MSU Student ’3 Alcohol Use Males. The Additive Model with perceptions of the average MSU student’s alcohol consumption was significant overall (38(4) = 18.20, p = .003, chox & 5m" = .09, Rzumm = .20) and had a successful prediction rate of 92%. The model did not show a block improvement over the Basic Model (x20) = .03, p = .862). The change in odds of drinking remained virtually the same as the Basic Model for the significant individual predictor of differentiation (OR = .190, 95% CI = .068, .530, see Table 31, p. 169). The Interaction Model also was significant overall (78(5) = 18.20, p = .003, R20». & 5nd1= .09, from“, = .20) and had a successful prediction rate of 92%. However, it did not show a block improvement over the Basic Model (x2(2) = .20, p = .900). Although the individual predictors of differentiation (OR = .181, 95% CI = .063, .525) and age (OR = 2.242, 95% CI = 1.227, 4.096) remained significant, the interaction term failed to reach a level of significance (see Table 31, p. 169). Females. The Additive Model with perceptions of the average MSU student’s alcohol consumption was not significant as an overall model (x2(4) = 7.004, p = .136, see Table 31, p. 169). The Interaction Model also was not significant as an overall model (38(5) = 7.480, p = .137, see Table 31, p. 169). I68 Hugo 2 rommwmo Womqommmo: $3833 mamas; 93 01:.» l $28303. om ch mamas» Cmo _ 3E8 _ medium _ a _ mm _ $5 _ ox _ 8.x. 9 _ o _ mm _ gen 9 ow 2 3.x. 9 wwmmo zona— Umrw 2.3 9% 293...... 97.5 93m 93¢ -93 98 92... 92c 9mg 93c 22 -92 93 98 923 922 :3 -93 o.oo. Pd 93a 93a _.o2 >mo 92 9.2 93...... 980 _.NwN arc-2 o.oo 93 9mm _.cmm 93m _.Axo >552?“ Zena 02-x 2.3 93 _98: 93c 98m 9H: -93 9mm “rub... 93o chow 90% W73 -92 93 98 923 930 :3 -98 92. who o.owu 93m _.ooo >mo 92 92 98.... NNE 2N: s: 3 98 93 93 _.oma 9qu _.Amm Emc C8 98 92 98 :5. 903 _.o; o.oo 92 o.oo 98c 90% _.oom 383030: 30%; 02-x Ln: 9% 92...... 922 98m 93m 9% 98 Puma 9M2 9N2 902 92 -98 93 93 90mm 93A :3 -93 92 no.2 9on 93m _.oom >mo 92 92 93...... when _.NN-N Poem 93 93 93 :8 98A _.2.» ch Cmo o.oo 92 95 _.ooe 93w _.ONA 98 92 92 o.ooo 93o _.ooo Um?” X ch CR -92 98 93 .09.. chow 2.8m -92 92 93 985 93a _.o: a. NA .8. ”So-8:8. .I. to A .o . "so-8:8. 169 Dzflerentiation & Perception of Best Friend at MS U ’3 Alcohol Use Males. The Additive Model with perceptions of their best friend at MSU’s alcohol consumption was significant overall (38(4) = 24.12, p < .001, R29,” 5n,“ = .11, [(2)-agent, = .27) with a significant block improvement over the Basic Model (x20) = 6. 12, p = .013), and had a successful prediction rate of 92.5%, an increase .5% over the Basic model. The change in odds for being a drinker remained a significant individual predictor of differentiation (OR = .225, 95% CI = .081, .624), when the student has an average level of age, BMI, and perception of their best friend at MSU’s alcohol use (see Table 32, p. 172). Similarly, when subjects had average levels of the other predictors, they were 1.02 times more likely to be a drinker with every increase of 1 unit in perceived monthly Quantity X Frequency drink they believed their friends drank. As the range for perceptions is quite large, a 1 unit change is relatively niiniscule. To better illustrate this effect, if a subject’s perception were to increase by the equivalent of one standard deviation for males (72.43), they would be 4.26 times more likely to be a drinker. Additionally, when differentiation and perceptions are at average levels, an individual is 2.27 times more likely to drink as they gain one year of age. The Interaction Model also was significant overall (38(5) = 24.64, p < .001, RIC” & Sue" = .12, Rznmm = .27) and had a successful prediction rate of 92.5% (see Table 32, p. 172); however, it did not show a block improvement over the Additive Model (xz( 1) = .52, p = .470). Although the interaction term itself was not significant, by adding it to the equation differentiation no longer significantly changed the odds of being a drinker. When the subject had average levels of the other predictors, they were 1.02 times more likely to be a drinker with every 1 unit increase of best friend perception and 1.75 times 170 for one standard deviation change. Additionally, when differentiation and age are at average levels, an individual is 2.28 times more likely to drink as they gain one year of age. Females. The Additive Model with perceptions of their best friend at MSU’s alcohol consumption was significant (38(4) = 28.53, p < .001, R20,x & Sm" = .11, Rznmmc = .20) with a significant block improvement over the Basic Model (x20) = 21.62, p < .001), and had miniscule improvement in successfirl prediction rate to 87%, an increase .4% over the Basic Model. Perceptions of best friend use changed the odds of being a drinker by 1.044 (95% CI = 1.018, 1.070) for every one unit change in perceptions (see Table 32, p. 172). Individuals who had higher best friend perception equivalent to one standard deviation for (41.48) were 5.95 times likely to be a Drinker versus Abstainer. Additionally, the presence of best fi'iend perceptions eliminates the significance of difl‘erentiation found in the Basic Model. The Interaction Model also was significant (36(5) = 29.48, p < .001, R29“ & Sm" = .1 1, [(2)-make = .21), but did not show a block improvement over the Additive Model (12(1) = .95, p = .329) and actually had a worse successful prediction rate (85.8%) than all of the other models—including not having a model at all. The interaction term was not significant, and the effect of perceptions of best fi'iend use remained equivalent to the Additive Model (OR = 1.044, 95% CI = 1.018, 1.070, see Table 32, p. 172). 171 333—3 3 homage womaommmos 383333 9:338 98 Oman .- woqoavmosm om mam” 35:: :8 3E3 woBEom @ rmm _ éma _ ow _ 8.x. 9 e _ mm _ €95 _ ow _ 3.x. 9 wwmmo Zona— UmH-W -_ .33 933 333...... 9 2 3 9333 93.3 -93 93 PB _. 93 3 9333 933 E": -92 93 93 933 9332 :3 -93 93... 3.33 933 9333 _.32 >mo 92 93 3.3.1. ~33 _.N3 P3— 93 93 933 _.33 93cm _.Amo >3333 93 93 3.33... 3.33 _.NZ N33 933 93 93 _.333 9333 _.3u wow” magma Cmo 93 92 P3... _.23 _.o2 2.33 93 92 2.3.... _.oi _.23 _.33 38398: 7330. 6mm.” -23 933 N2 . 9qu 933 2.33 -933 93 92 903 933 3.33 E": -92 93 92 903 9333 :3 -933 93 _.NA 9033 9mm— _.33 >mo 93 93 3.3... ~33 _.N: 3.33 933 9_ 3 93 _.333 9333 _.33 wow" magnum Cma 93 92 93... 2.23 930 2.33 93 92 2.3 _ a... 2.33 _.2 w _.o3c Um?” X mama mania Cmo 92 92 93 2.2— 933 2.03 93 93 _.oo _.33 .2: .32 HA .3. "so-82am. _._... 3 A .2. 25-833. 172 Diflerentiatian & COA Status Males. The Additive Model with COA status was significant overall (38(4) = 24.09, p < .001, R20»- & 5...," = .11, Rzumumc = .27) with a significant block improvement over the Basic Model (380) = 6.07, p = .014).The model had a successful prediction rate of 92%, the same'as the Basic Model. The individual predictor of differentiation remained significant (OR = .162, 95% CI = .052, .507, see Table 33, p. 174) when the student has an average level of age, BMI, and was a COA. Being a COA by itself did not increase the odds of being classified as a drinker; however this may have more to do with the sample. The results of these analyses should be interpreted as controlling for COA status, as none of the Abstainers had an alcoholic parent. The Interaction Model was also significant as an overall model (12(5) = 24.07, p < .001, R20,” 5...,“ = .11, Rzumm = .27) and had a successful prediction rate of 92%; however, it did not show a block improvement over the Additive Model (x2( I) = .000, p = 1.000). The interaction term was neither significant nor did it change the significant odds for differentiation (OR = .162, 95% CI = .052, .507, see Table 33, p. 174). Females. The Additive Model with COA status was not significant as an overall model (76(4) = 8.064, p = .089, see Table 33, p. 174). The Interaction Model also failed to achieve a significance as an overall model (36(5) = 8.200, p = .146, see Table 33, p. 174). Dlfierentiation & Expectations of Alcohol Serving as a Tension Reducer Males. The Additive Model with expectations of alcohol as a tension reducer was significant overall (x2(4) = 55.06, p < .001, R2 Cox & Snell = .24, Rznmmc = .56) With a 173 H35 8 rommmno ”08638: 33338 £338 92 015* I 00> mSEm _ K38 _ moBEom _ u mm H 53 _ ox _ 3.x. 9 _ w _ mm _ 2% _ ox _ 8.x. 9 W83 goua— Ume Lao 98 3.8.... 93¢ .88 98¢ -93 98 PM: 93o 9n8 93o ESH -93 93 98 980 .83 :3 -98 98 8.3 93a 93o _.oS >mo 9M: 9”: o.oo: 9N3 _.Num “#3— 98 93 98 :88 93m #36 253$” gono— Umzw Law 98 93.... 93w bum 9m3 -93 98 9mm... 93m 98o 993 El: o.oo 93 o.oo 98a .83 :3 -98 98 9% 908 983 _.oou >ma 9m— 93 3.8a: Nbam _.Num Nr30 98 93 9mm _.38 938 #33 00> L 98 8.3.3 98 o.ooo o.ooo . 98 9.3 _.B _.3 o 98¢ Pm _ 8 388030: 30%; Um?” 93 98 93...... 93m 98m 9m3 -93 98 .28... 93m 9~Nw o.ooo wz: 98 93 o.oo 998 983 :3 -98 92 PE 93a 93a _.oom >mo 9M: 93 3.3: NNAM _.Nuw Kr3o 98 9 3 9w; _.8_ 938 _.38 00> - _ 93 83.8 o.oo o.ooo o.ooo . 93 9.3 #8 fat 93A 983 Um?” x 00> 53 33.3 o.oo Pm; o.ooo . 98 93 9E _.uuc 9N3 93o .6 A .8, «so-E39 IN A .3. arc-8:3. 174 significant block improvement over the Basic Model (12(1) = 37.06, p < .001), and had successfill prediction rate of 95.5%, an increase 3.5% over the Basic model. The tension reduction variable was significant as subjects were 1.388 (95% CI = 1.175, 1.639) times more likely to be a Drinker with a 1 unit increase in tension reduction expectations (see Table 34, p. 176). Subjects whose expectations were by one standard deviation greater (8.73) are 17.53 times more likely to be drinkers. The Interaction Model also was significant overall (36(5) = 55.73, p < .001, R20. & 5..." = .24, Rznmlkm = .57) and had a successful prediction rate of 95.5%; however, it did not show a block improvement over the Additive Model (x20) = .67, p = .412). The interaction term did not significantly change the odds of being a drinker and the tension reduction variable maintained the same level of influence (OR = 1.375, 95% CI = 1.169, 1.618, see Table 34, p. 176). Females. The Additive Model with tension reduction expectations was significant as an overall model (12(4) = 81 .39,p < .001, R20,” 5..." = .28, [Pp-mm.“ = .52) and had a significant block improvement over the Basic Model (x2(1)= 74.47, p < .001). The model had a successful prediction rate of 91 .5%, an increase of nearly 5% over the Basic model. Expectations of alcohol serving as a tension reducer changed the odds of being a drinker by 1.374 (95% CI = 1.225, 1.542) for a one unit difference in expectations (see Table 34, p. 176). By changing the difference in expectations to a unit equivalent to one standard deviation for expectations (8.8.1) the likelihood of being a Drinker versus abstainer is 16.47 times greater. Additionally, the presence of tension reduction expectations eliminated the significance of differentiation found in the Basic Model. 175 Hugo wk rommmzo flamingo: 3.33:5 magma :5: Oman I mxvgfimmoaw cm H258: flamenco: 3&8 _ moBEam a _ mm _ éaa _QC 89.9 a a 4 mm_ 55 _ow_ 89.9 mean 7.3%.; cm; -98 9a _92: 93¢ 98“ 93° -93 98 92.. 9m; 98“ 93° wz: -92 93 98 926 $2 :3 -98 92 9d 98a 93a _.9: £0 92 93 98: Sue Cu» .8: 98 9; 9g 5% 38 _.ae >858 Zena cm; 9% 9% 98 933 9:: 93a -9; 9; 9: 9me 9§ $3 wz: 93 98 98 _.So 93... 3% -98 9E 93 93“ 38 SS 2% 93 93 38.. 93.. _.Se 93... 9; 98 9.5 _.E 95 _.Sa 38%: 98:88 9% 98 2.8: 5% Ed 88 98 98 N93: 3: _.BM 33 58338: Keno— Um; 9s GM 98 ~95 98” 3.83 9% _.8 93 38 9M: .38 BE 92 98 9o. 28 93° :3 -98 99. 9.; 9e: 935 2% £0 9% 9% 98°. 83 _._: SB 9: 98 93 _.Ge 9§ 3% #8389888: 93 98 :3: Cd 2% Z; 93 98 N92: 33 SB 9%.. 3:; .318 98:38 9: 9; 9E _._; 993 it 98 98 9.3 2% 933 _.qu .. an A .8. 30.823. 2.. Ne A .2. "Ea-8:8. 176 The Interaction Model also was significant overall (12(5) = 81.82, p < .001, R20... & 5nd1= .28, Rznwde = .52) and had a successful prediction rate of 91 .1%; however, it did not show a block improvement over the Additive Model (780) = .43, p = .511). The interaction term was not significant and the significant odds ratio for tension reduction remained equivalent to the Additive Model (OR = 1.370, 95% CI = 1.223, 1.534, see Table 34, p. 176). Ditfirentiation & Expectations of Alcohol Serving as a Social Lubricant Males. The Additive Model with expectations of alcohol as a social lubricant was significant overall (38(4) = 53.38, p < .001, 12%,, s s..." = .23, [(2ch = .55) with a significant block improvement over the Basic Model (x20) = 35.28, p < .001). The model had a successful prediction rate of 95.5%, an increase 3.5% over the Basic model. The social lubrication variable was significant for subjects as individuals with 1 unit higher social lubrication expectations were 1.366 (95% CI = 1.177, 1.586) times greater odds of being a Drinker (see Table 35, p. 179). Subjects whose expectations were one standard deviation (7.89) higher were 11.73 times more likely to be a drinker. The Interaction Model also was significant overall (38(5) = 53.96, p < .001, R2c°x & 5.," = .24, from = .55) and had a successful prediction rate of 95.5%; however, it did not show a block improvement over the Additive Model (x20) = .68, p = .410). The interaction term did not significantly change the odds of being a Drinker versus Abstainer and the social lubricant variable maintained the same level of influence (OR = 1.361, 95% CI = 1.174, 1.577, see Table 35, p. 179) Females. The Additive Model with social lubrication expectations was significant as an overall model (36(4) = 66.75, p < .001, R2Cox ,, s..." = .24, mm“, = .44) and had a 177 significant block improvement over the Basic Model (380) = 59.84, p < .001). The model had a successful prediction rate of 89.4%, an increase of nearly 3% over the Basic Model. Expectations of alcohol serving as a social lubricant changed the odds of being a drinker by 1.387 (95% CI = 1.227, 1.567) for every one unit change in perceptions (see Table 35, p. 179).1By altering the unit of change to be equivalent to one standard deviation for expectations (7.74) students were 12.58 times more likely to be a Drinker versus an Abstainer Additionally, the presence of social lubrication expectations eliminates the significance of differentiation found in the Basic Model. The Interaction Model also was significant overall (386) = 68.81, p < .001, R29», & s..." = .24, 1?sz = .45) and had a successful prediction rate of 90.7%; however, it did not show a block improvement over the Additive Model (x2(1)= .43, p = .511). The interaction term was not significant and the significant odds ratio for social lubrication became 1.418 (95% CI = 1.239, 1.623) for a one unit change in expectations (see Table 35, p. 179). The Comprehensive Model Males. The Comprehensive Model was significant according to the model chi- square (1203) = 66.18, p < .001, RICO... 3,... = .28, RIM“, = .66) and showed a significant improvement over the Basic Model (x2(10)= 48.18,p < .001). The model illustrated a 96% success rate in predicting the correct drinking classifications, an improvement of 4% over the Basic Model. Interestingly, taken as a whole the model was significant; however, no individual predictors in this model significantly changed the odds of being a drinker (see Table 36, p. 181). 178 Haw—n um rommmao Womaommmoa vamamoaam mammam 9m: 01:.» l men—688325 om momma. rewind—mo: _l 3&8 _ man—5&8 _ a _ mm _ 9...: _ ow _ 89.2 _ a :m _ fie: _ ow _ 39.9 woman Zeno. Umzw -_ .3 9mm _931 9 _ co 93m 9mg -93 93 PB a 9m _ o 92% 930 W7: -93 93 98 90% 9mg :3 -93 9g and 98a 93a 92: >mo 9M: 9“: 98...... wk? 9an Po: 9% 9 3 9mm _.omm 9.3m _.Amo >335 zona— UwEw -93 9mm 93 9mg 9N8 _.owm 93 93 _.ow 93. 998 No? 95 98 9cm 9 _ m 995 93o :8 -98 93 9B 928 98A _.oow >9». 9% 9mm moo... thw :3 Memo 93 9 _ m “a: _.Nfi 93w 9qu mean: 931820: 9B 98 3.9»: _.uam :3 _.mma 9mm. 9cm 3.3.... _.wmq _.qu _.mou 583830: Zona— UmTW 92 fun 98 _.muo 95k 398 _.mo 93 Nam Agoo 9.2: .365 $5 98 9on 9_ m 989 93m :8 -93 92 93 923 90: _.3o >mo 93 93 mama... NS— :mo mp3 93 9; :5 _.NS 93m _.quo mega b.5183: 9w— 9om 360...... _.wE _._.E _.m: 93 93 ~32: _.fim _.Nwo 9an Um?” X mega 951838 9: 95 9d :3. 93m _.3m 93 93 NS _._ma 902 _.AS _._ me A bu. «Seaman. _._... N» A .2. 35-69%? 179 Females. The Comprehensive Model was significant according to the model chi- square (11203) = 90.70, p < .001, chmm = .31, Raw...“ = .57) and showed a significant improvement over the Basic Model (x2(10)= 83.79, p < .001). The model illustrated a 91 .5%'success rate in predicting the correct drinking classifications, an improvement of almost 5% over the Basic Model. Only the tension reduction variable showed an individual level of significance in changing the odds of being a Drinker (OR = 1.237, 95% CI = 1.078, 1.419, see Table GAU 36, p. 181). Moderate Drinkers versus Low Drinkers Difi'erentiation and GA U Males. The Basic Model did not reach a level of significance (£0) = 2.92, p = .405, see Table 37, p. 182). Females. The Basic Model failed to reach a level of significance (12(3) = 2.62, p = .453, see Table 37, p. 182). F our factors of the DSI-R with GA U Males. As was the case with the Basic Model, the overall four-factor model failed to reach a level of significance (12(6) = 4.94, p = 551, see Table 38, p. 184). Females. As was the case in the Basic Model, the four factor model failed to reach a level of significance as an overall model (78(6) = 2.97, p = .813, see Table 38, p. l 84). Dmerenfiafion & perceptions of the average MS U student ’s alcohol use. Males. The Additive Model with perceptions of the average MSU student’s alcohol consumption was not significant (78(4) = 8.655, p = .070, see Table 39, p. 184). The Interaction Model also failed to reach a level of significance (38(5) = 9.94, p = .077). 180 H33 ”3 roman." womnommmoa vaamnnmsm manoaa 9m: Claw 1 OoBuansmmé Zona— Zaom mesa—am 3 mm 935 O” 3.x. Q 3 mm «Sea 0% 8.x. 0: Um?” -93 9va _.co 93m 9 _ 3 :38 _._u _._ _ _ .3 u. _ a 933 N930 ES: 9 a 93 N3 :3 93m _.Aco -93 93 93 903 oboa _.omm >mo 93 93 9 _ q _.o3 93mm _.oNN 9 _ N 9: 93 :3 933 :63 Km: $083308 o.oo 93 93 9000 903 _.SN 98 9E 93 98m 93m _.o: 0.2% X 25:: $28283 -93 9o— wbw 93a 98o roam 98 92 93 _.oow 93.). _.SN mom. mini 98 9c: :93: roma _.SA _.oum 93 90: NS _.3 w 963 _.oS Ume X mam: 339$ -98 92 93 98m 93m _.oa 9c: 93 9G _.o3 903 2:3 00> ._ . _ a 93 who 9w : u 93: _.3a 93 9% 9mm _.ucu 9.39 A.Aoa Umrw X 00> _.3 _._m #3 9.33 933 3.58 93 90A 98 _._: 95m NAB H258: Wane—nae: 93 9cm 9% _.o3 98m :3 9: 93 93...... _.N3 _.3m _.Sc Um?” X #958: £89030: 9: 9: 90¢ _._ _m 93: f3» 93 95 9o: _.o3 9qu _.NNN moon: reggae: -93 93 93 92m 93m _.omm 9 _ w 93 93 _._AN 923 _.wwa Um?” X moni— rccaomaoa -93 93 9mm 93. 93a _.NOu 93 93. 9.8 :3 93¢ _.wf _._ h A .3. 23-823. .1. E A .3. 35-823. l8l .735 3 rommmmn ”$835: 33533 952.8 9% m3 3053.8 585 hos Una—83 I Diana—Emacs 7.5.8 mama—am 5 mm 9&5 OX 8:. Q 5 mm 9&5 OW 3.x. 0— 0m?” -9 _ a 9X 98 9mm~ 9&3 _.58 -98 9mm 9mm 9.33 93o _.Aou E5 -98 98 N8 908 98c _.8o -98 98 95 93m 908 :5; >ma -98 9 _ m 93 90mm 938 _.88 9- 9 _ m N. _ 3 .9: 98: ~58 Co A .8. 35-8555. _._... h A .2. 3.0-855. .365 um rommmao ”£835: 5.35535 9525.. 9m” 86 2559.98 338 has Una—83 I mos. $888 755m moaaom 5 mm 9&5 OX 8.x. 9 5 mm 9&5 OW 8.x. 9 magma—5. ”83758. -98 98— 9; 93.3 .39 .58 -98 9N5 9S 93m 98m _.53 J: Page: 98 98— 93 _.u _o 9.33 958 98 93 98 room 9mm~ ~.3o magmas. 028mm -93 95 _.3o 938 93...” Sec -93 9N9 93 985 9me _.fiS F555 2:: 0924. 9; 93 93 :3 938 N35 -93 98m .33 98m 953. .58 Eé AGE 98 N. _ o 908 935 _.omc -98 98 .80 98A 908 _.3N >mo 38m _.mo 98 908 938 _.u 3 9- 9 _ m N. _ om _.Nau 98w _.amm c. A .8. 25-5.3. _._. a A .8. 3928. 182 Females. The Additive Model with perceptions of MSU students’ alcohol consumption failed to reach a level of significance (12(4) = 3.77, p = .43 8, see Table 39, p. 184). The Interaction Model also failed to reach a level of significance (36(5) = 4.63, p = .463, see Table 39, p. 184). Diflerentiation & Perception of Best Friend at MSU 's Alcohol Use Males. The Additive Model with perceptions of their best friend at MSU’s alcohol consumption was significant (12(4) = 19.894 p = .001, R2“, & 5,... = .15, RIM“, = .20) with a significant block improvement over the Basic Model (12(1) = 16.98, p < .001). The model had successful prediction rate of 68.8%, an increase of greater than 18% over the Basic Model. Perception of best fi'iend alcohol use was a significant individual predictor in that the odds of being a Moderate Drinker were 1.024 (95% CI = 1.011, 1.037) times greater (see Table 40, p. 186) for every one unit change in perception and 5.69 times greater for a change of 1 standard deviation (72.43). The Interaction Model also was significant overall (x2(5) = 21.2l, p < .001, R20”, & 5...," = .16, RZNW = .21) and had a successful prediction rate of 68.0%, however, it did not show a block improvement over the Additive Model (380) = 1.32, p = .251). Perception of best friend alcohol use was a significant individual predictor in that the odds of being a Moderate Drinker were 1.025 (95% CI = 1.011, 1.039, see Table 40, p. 186). 183 Hugo we rommmao anaommmoa 588:8 mange 32 m8 Zone-38 <02; r02 013.83 I $908325 o»- ch wagon: Cmo f 3&8 _ moan—om _ a _ mm _ sea _ ow _ ease _ a _ mm 2 so: ow _ ease 2520381 Umzw -9 3 9?» 98 938 9&8 _.afi. -98 98 98 9.38 980 2&8 wz: -98 98 8.8 98m 98o 28¢ -98 9?. 98¢ 93m o.ooo _.oma. >mo -98 93 98 98m 9.5m _.qu 98 9G 8.: _.83 98— _.mmw >aa5mo -98 9 _ a 98 903 9a _ w _.wi 92 9 2 m 2.0m _.8m 98c _.80 Ewe Cmo 98 92 P8... 2.2m .28 _.8o 92 92 2.8 _.ooa 93m _.2m 58328: Zona— DMEN -9 _ a 93 9 _ c 93m 98; 2.8a. -98 98 9.3 93o 98A :28 get: -98 98 _.3 925 988 2.80 -98 98 9 I 923 9ch _.8N >ma -92 9 a o.oo 908 98. 2.3» 98 9_ m who _.NAm 908 _.od ch CR 92 92 +3... _.25 _.o2 _.8w 92 92 _.Nm _.ooa 98m _.2m 02-x x ch 92 92 _.Nm _.oE 93c _.80 -92 92 93 908 93a _.ooc Cma _._ E A bus "So-8:3. I. w A .2. sec-£59 Females. The Additive Model with perceptions of their best friend at MSU’s alcohol consumption was significant (x2(4) = 19.29, p = .001, RZCM a 5...,“ = .13, [Pram-1km = .17) with a significant block improvement over the Basic Model (12(1) = 16.67, p < .001) and a successful prediction rate of 68.3%, an increase of 6.3% over the Basic model. The change in odds for being a Moderate Drinker versus a Low Drinker was 1.025 (95% CI = 1.010, 1.040) for every 1 unit change in perception of best friend drinking, and 2.71 times for an increase equivalent to one standard deviation (41.48) (see Table 40, p. 186). ' The Interaction Model was also significant overall (38(5) = 19.55, p = .002, 18%,, & 3m" = .13, [gum = .18) and had a successful prediction rate of 68.3%; however, it did not show a block improvement over the Additive Model (x20) = .26, p = .611). The only significant individual predictor was perception of best fi'iend use, which remained identical to the value of the Additive Model (see Table 40, p. 186). Dyfirentiation & C 0A Status Males. The Additive Model with COA status did not reach a level of significance (78(4) = 2.92, p = .571, see Table 41, p. 187). The Interaction Model also failed to reach a level of significance 06(5) = 2.94, p = .709). Females. The Additive Model with COA status failed to reach a level of significance 08(4) = 2.88, p = .578, see Table 41, p. 187). The Interaction Model also failed to reach a level of significance 086) = 3.218, p = .666). 185 .320 .5 rommmzo womaammmoa 082038 250:8 Em" man 38038 588 P03 Can—83 1 0280225 ow wow” magma Cmo _ 330m _ moan—ow b a 2 mm _ an: _ ow _ 8.x. 9 _ a _ mm _ fin: _ ox _ 3.x. 9 mean Kong 02-” -9 3 98A 98 9w8 9&8 r98 -98 98 98 908 98c _.A8 ES— -98 90¢ 8.8 98m 980 28¢ -98 98 98a 908 908 _.oaA >mo -98 93 98 90mm 908 _.woM 98 93 8.3 2.82 902 _.amu >a&:8 -9 8 9 2 0 93 98A 980 _.88 93 9 _ a T3 _.Noa 980 _.8a mom” manna; Cmo 98 92 8.88.. _.8A 2b: _.80 98 92 5.8.... _.8m 22 o _.vo 383030: 30%; 62.” 98 93 9.: TBA 93m ALE -98 93 9E 908 98o _.mm0 22 -98 98 2.3 98o 928 :08 -98 93. 93 90mm 9me :50 >8 -98 9; 93 980 982 _.88 9; 93 83 :8 988 _.80 wow» man—ad Cmo 98 92 8.8.... 2.98 _.o: 2.80 98 92 0.8: _.8m _.oo0 no.5 UmH-W x wow» 3.225% Cmo 92 92 _.N-N 2.2a. 900o _.ouw -92 92 98m 908 908 _.8o ... h A .8. "So-$259 3.8 A .2. "So-8:09 186 0.033 t rommmmo womnommmos 3003005 mange 0.5” Ed 7003088 338 has Una—88 I 00> mgcm _ 3&8 _ 02:38 r 0 _ mm _ €33 _ 2w _ 03$ 2 _ 3 _ mm _ng _ 0% F 03.x. 2 wmmmo Kama— Umzw -0. 3 0.80 0.8 0.08 9.38 _.38 -98 0.8 0.8 0808 .80 :38 ES. -98 0.03 8.8 0.083 0.80 _.80 -98 0.00 983 0.08 .000 203A >ma -98 0.3 0.0-\ 0.030 088 _.803 0.8 0.3 8.: _.Nfi .0”: _.38 >035mo -900 9 2 3 98 0.030 088 _.803 0.8 9 _ 3 8.8 _.83 980 2.33— OO> -900 0.3. 92 0.033 938 8.303 98 9.3 983 _.83_ 0.83 8.30 258—Samoa 3002 szw -93 0.8 0.8 908 933 2.80 -98 0.8 0.03 .300 0.80 0.93 wz: -98 0.03 2 .03 0.083 0.80 280 -98 90¢ 900 .08 0.00» 2 .030 >mo -98 0.3 0.8 0.030 088 _.803 0.8 0.3 8.”: _.N3_ 908 2.33 00> -98 932 900 0.000 988 N38 93 0.03 900 _.3_ 0.33 8.08 Um?” x 00> -9: 0.2 0.8 9000 9:8 0.08 0.3 0.8 0.8 0.300 0.88 0.033 *8 A .03v 25.880. 1.0 A .2. "so-8:03. 187 Difizrentiation & Expectations of Alcohol Serving as a Tension Reducer Males. The Additive Model with expectations of alcohol as a tension reducer was significant (78(4) = 16.17, p = .003, RZCM 5,... = .12, mm = .16) with a significant block improvement over the Basic Model (38(1) = 13.26, p < .001), and had successful prediction rate of 69.6%, an increase 19.2% over the Basic Mode]. The tension reduction variable was significant indicating subjects were 1.101 (95% CI = 1.042, 1.163) times more likely of being a moderate drinker versus a low drinker with a 1 unit increase in tension reduction expectations (see Table 42, p. 189). Subjects whose expectations changed by one standard deviation (8.73) were 2.31 times more likely to be drinkers. The Interaction Model was also significant overall (78(5) = 16.24, p = .006, 12%,, & 5.,“ = .12, RZNM = .16) and had a successful prediction rate of 70.4%; however, it did not show a block improvement over the Additive Model (x20) = .07 , p = .798). The significant tension reduction odds ration was identical to the Additive Model (see Table 42, p. 189). Females. The Additive Model with expectations of alcohol as a tension reducer was significant (36(4) = 24.47, p < .001, R2“, ,1 s..." = .16, hm, = .22) with a significant block improvement over the Basic Model (36(1) = 21 .84, p < .001), and a successful prediction rate of 66.9%, an increase of 5% over the Basic model. Age (OR= 1.386, 95% CI = 1.007, 1.90) and Tension reduction (OR = 1.123, 95% C1 = 1.064, 1.184 for a 1 unit change, and OR = 2.78 for a change of one standard deviation of 8.81 units, see Table 42, p. 189). .320 AM rommmmo Wow—dame: vanamoasm 953$ 92 m3 30%-.88 588 float 013.83 I wax—808205 0». H253: flamenco: T 3&8 mania _ o _ mm _ 55 _ ox _ 8&9 s m a: 2% _ ow. _ 3:6. momma Zone. Ume -9 3 9.3 98 93m 9th. _.afi -98 93 93 933 93o _.Aou W75 -93 93 Now 993 93c _.owo -98 93 93 93m 925 93¢ >mn -93 93 93 93w 93w :3 9mm 93 N: _.Nfi 9o”: _.3u >338 Zona— Ume 92 93 93 _.N3 920 Name 93 93 9B :3 93-3 ~33 wz: -93 93 93 93; 93c _.o-Z -93 93 o.oo 903 933 2:3 >mo -93 9 m q 93 9on 933 awe 93 9 _ a Pom... :3 foo-V _.oom Adamo: W885: 95 93 Susan :3 _.ofi :3 9E 93 5.3:. :3 r3» :3 388030: 30%: Ume 98 9mm 93 _.Nwo 933 ~68 93 93 9: :3 93w 9wa W7: -93 93 93 93c 93c _.oqo -93 93 93 923 93m roam >mo -93 9 _ a 93 930 9me _.ufio 9mm 9 H m Pow... _.umu “boa _.cou 4253: W883: 98 93 2.: a... :2 _.oAN :3 9: 93 3.8.3.. :8 r3: :3 02% x Anzac: x8830: -93 93 93. 903 93c #23 -98 93 9N5 93a 93— _.o: am A .3. ”fie-gnu. _._... No A .3. 25-839 The Interaction Model also was significant overall (36(5) = 24.7l,p < .001, RZCO, & 5....- = .16, 32,-W.,, = .22) but failed to yield a significant block improvement over the Additive Model (x20) = .24, p = .622), maintaining the identical prediction rate of the Additive Model 66.9%. Age (OR= 1.383, 95% CI = 1.006, 1.903) and tension reduction (OR = 1.121, 95% CI = 1.061, 1.183 for al unit change) remained nearly identical in their respective values of categorical prediction between Moderate versus Low Drinkers (see Table 42, p. 189). Diflerentiation & Expectations of Alcohol Serving as a Social Lubricant Males. The Additive Model with expectations of alcohol as a social lubricant approached, but did not obtain, significance (12(4) = 9.37, p = .052, see Table 43, p. 193). Additionally, the Interaction Model also was not significant (x2(5) = 9.386, p = .095). Females. The Additive Model with expectations of alcohol as a social lubricant was significant overall (38(4) = 14.01, p = .007, R’CM 5....- = .10, RZNW, = .13) with a significant block improvement over the Basic Model (38(1) = 11.38, p = .001), and a successful prediction rate of 61 .2%, which was .7% less than the Basic Model. The social lubrication variable was a significant individual predictor, indicating subjects had a 1.094 (95% CI = 1.036, 1.155) times greater chance of being classified as a Moderate Drinker versus Low Drinker with a 1 unit increase in social lubrication (see Table 43, p. 193). Subjects whose expectations change by one standard deviation (7 .74) were 2.01 times more likely to be Moderate Drinkers. 190 The Interaction Model was also significant (x2(5) = 14.18, p = .015, RICO“ SM“ = .10, 1?sz = .13) and had a successful prediction rate of 61 .2%. The model did not have significant block improvement over the Additive Model (380) = .171 , p = .679). Social lubrication remained the only significant predictor and the values remained equivalent to the Additive Model (OR = 1.095, 95% CI = 1.037, 1.157, see Table 43, p. 193). The Comprehensive Model Males. The Comprehensive Model was significant according to the model chi- square (38(13) = 34.99, p = .001, RIC,” s..." = .24, RIM“, = .33) and showed a significant improvement over the Basic Model (3800) = 32.07, p < .001). The model illustrated a 73.6% success rate in predicting the correct drinking classifications, an improvement of 13.2% over the Basic Model. Perceptions of best friend use (OR = 1.020) and expectations of tension reduction (OR = 1.109) were the only individual variables to significantly improve the odds of being classified as a Moderate versus a Low Drinker (see Table 44, p. 194). Females. The Comprehensive Model was significant according to the model chi- square (x203) = 45.72, p < .001, RICO, & s...“ = .28, mm = .38) and showed a significant improvement over the Basic Model (7800) = 43.1, p < .001). The model illustrated a 75.5% success rate in predicting the correct drinking classifications, an improvement of 13.6% over the Basic Model. Age (OR = 1.461, 95% CI = 1.014, 2.107), perceptions of best friend use (OR = 1.028, 95% CI = 1.009, 1.047), and expectations of tension reduction (OR = 1.118, 95% CI = 1.041, 1.201) were all significant individual 19] predictors in the likelihood of being classified as a Moderate versus Low Drinker (see Table 44, p. 194). High Drinkers versus Low Drinkers Differentiation and GA U Males. The Basic Model did not reach a level of significance according to the model chi-square (38(3) = 2.208, p = .530, see Table 45, p. 195). Females. The Basic Model failed to reach a level of significance (38(3) = 2.057, p = .561, see Table 45, p. 195). F our factors of the DSI-R with GA U Males. As was the case in the basic model, the overall four-factor model failed to reach a level of significance (x2(6) = 10.841, p = .093, see Table 46, p. 195). Females. The four factor model failed to reach a level of significance as an overall model (36(6) = 3.405, p = .757, see Table 46, p. 195). Dlflerentiation & perceptions of the average MSU student ’s alcohol use Males. The Additive Model with perceptions of the average MSU student’s alcohol consumption was significant as an overall model for predicting High versus Low Drinkers (38(4) = 24.11, p < .001, 1&0, ,, 3,... = .18, 12sz == .24). The Additive Model had a successful prediction rate of 65%, a 9.7% improvement over the Basic Model. Only perceptions of MSU student use was a significant individual predictor in the Additive Model indicating that for every 1 unit increase in perceptions they were 1.032 (95% CI = 1.016, 1.049) times more likely to be classified as a High Drinker (see Table 47, p. 197). With a one standard deviation change (37.22), the likelihood for being a Moderate Drinker was 3.29 times greater. 192 .320 3 rommmmo Wamammmo: 3.383% 9:938 52 m8 gonna?” 53% was 01288 I mxnoogmoam o». magm— 551330: Zion _ mango—am e _ mm _ fie: ow _ 8:9 _ e _ mm _ éea _ ox _ 8&9 wmmmo Zona— Umrw -o. _ o out. 93 Paw chew _.fiw -obu Pug. ohm 9ch 93¢ _.Aow W7: .98 o.oo Now 908 93¢ _.owc .98 PE oho 0.3m o.ooo _.oi >mo .9?“ o. _ a 93 Paw Paw _.wom ohm o. _ m N. _ .V _.N.: 0.8. raw >aa5mo .92 93 93 993 92: _.wuc PS 93 Pow _.w8 o.oam ruqm mesa r5185: o.ou o.ou «o.oo... _.omo 2:.“ :3 o.oo 98 3.3.1. _.ooA _.Sa :3 58830: Kong 62% 9.5 0.3 93 _._NA 003 Pwuo PS Pun ohm _.Noa on: NUS 9’4: .98 o.oo _.3 903 SWAN _.omm -93 93 PS Poem o.ooo rowm >Mo -obp o. _ a 98 o.oo”. o.oo» _.wwo 93 o. m m Pow _.w _ o 903 _.SN moan. 551035: 93 o.ow 93%.. _.ome mo: :8 o.oo 0.8 3.3.... _.oom rowq :3 Ume x moor: 551820: 0 3 93 o.om _.ooa o.owo _._oo PS 93 93 EoN— o.owo :NA .. e A .8. gamed. the A .8. 398:8. I93 32o t homage womaommmos 32:03.8 meqoam :5” Rd 33088 33% mos Una—81o. l OoBvarasmmé Zona— Zm—nm moan—am 3 mm £35 3w 8.x. 3 @ mm («SE 3w 8.x. 3 03-x 98¢ 98 93 _.83 98.3 98¢ -98 9mm 98 980 983 Nmae 3": -98 98 9mg 98o 98w _.omm -98 98 9% 98¢ 98A _.omw >ma -98 9 _ c 98 988 98m 3.88 98 9 3 c a. _ 8.. _.AS _.35 N. _8 78¢ $28388 93 93 PB 3.3u 93a #80 93 93 98 _.oco 908 _.3m U3-” X ZmC 30283303 93 93 9~a _.ooq 93c _.80 -93 93 _.3 98m 90% _.3o mam” man?— 98 93 93...... _.8c 3.8 _.8m 98 93 93... _._ _.owm _.ooo Tog Um?” x mam" manna 93 93 98 _.ooa 903 _.8_ -93 98 9%. 98¢ 93o _.o3 00> -9 _ N 93 98 9me 980 u. 3 mm -98 98 98 90mm 983 Na 3 m U3-” x 00> 980 3.8 95 _.Sm 93w $03 93 98 9AM _.mcu 935 5.03 Adamo: ”88:0: 93 98 93... :8 he: _.Noc 9: 98 98...... _._ _m 3?: _.N3 Um?” x H258: ”3:38: -98 98 93 98a 938 :8 -95 93 8.3 A53 9.3m _.3m mega gang—no: -93 98 98 983 908 _.oma 98 98 98 _ .8 _ 98o :8 Um?” x moor: rains—mo: 98 o.oo 98 E83 938 _.Bm 9: 98 _.8 _._ 3 98m _.88 _._ E A .8. 35.823. _._... w A .3. "so-8:3. 194 Hugo 3 homage Womnommmoa wnoamoasm mzaoam 93 m8 Em: 83% roé 033.82 I baggage: 2E8 mafia—8 @ mm éma OW 8.x. Q v mm $35 0% 8.x. 0— Umzw -98 93 _.mm 93m 98.; _.NoM -98 93 93 993 939 ..A8 W7: o.oo 98 93 foo» 98o _.oea -98 9i _.8 98¢ o.ooo _.oaA >mo o.oo 9 3 98m _.oou 93A :30 9mm 9 3 m 93 926 98_ _.omu _...u A .8, "So-Swag. {Nu A .3, "so-8:8. Haw—a .3 roman.“ Womammmo: qumomsm magma 92 Em: 585.. r92 Una—88 .- «.2: 3883 3E8 maafiom v mm «.33 OW 3.x. 3 v mm 9&5 OW 83 Q maomosa ”$035 9% 98 9mm _.wao 98» PM: 93 9mm 98 3.8” 9.38 N28 .4: $850: -93 98 98...... 93» 9qu 9mg 93 98 98 #80 93“ _.m: mac-585E 053% -98 9mm 93 93c 9&3 83 -98 93 93 93c 9m£ _.mmo 353: SE. 0908 -93 93 93 923 936 :8 .93 98 3.8 93.» 9on Home wz: 98 98 9S 33¢ 930 _._ 5 -93 98 3.3 9?: chug _.oww >mo 9cm 9 _ u 98 _.oqw 9m3 3.3. -93 9 _ A o.oo 93; 9d” 88 _._ h A .omv "So-3:8. _I w A .3, "So-8:3. 195 The Interaction Model also was significant as an overall model for predicting high versus low drinkers (36(4) = 24.48, p < .001, RZCM 5,... = .13, mm“, = .24) and had a 65.9% successful prediction rate. However, the Interaction Model did not show a block improvement over the Additive Model (38(2) = .37, p = .545). The only significant individual predictor was the perception of MSU students use, which remained almost identical to the OR from the Additive Model (1.032, 95% CI = 1.015, 1.049, see Table 47, p. 197). Females. The Additive Model with perceptions of their best friend at MSU’s alcohol consumption was significant (12(4) = 13.42, p = .009, 1620,x & 5...," = .09, RZNW = .11) and a significant block improvement over the Basic Model (12(1) = 11.36, p = .001), and had successful prediction rate of 60.7%, an increase of greater than 8% over the Basic Model. Perception of MSU student use was a significant individual predictor in that the likelihood was 1.015 (95% CI = 1.006, 1.025) times greater of being a High versus Low Drinker for every one unit change in perception (see Table 47, p. 197) and 1.79 times greater for every one standard deviation (38.75). The Interaction Model also was significant overall (12(5) = 18.01, p = .003, chOx & 5m" = .11, 1(2an = .15), had a successful prediction rate of 62.7%, and had a significant block improvement over the Additive Model (12(1) = 4.59, p = .032). Perception of MSU student use remained a significant predictor of High versus Low Drinkers (OR = 1.015, 95% CI = 1.006, 1.025, see Table 47, p. 197). Additionally, the interaction of differentiation with perception of MSU student use was significant with the 196 Hugo e3 rommmzo Womamwmos 309035 manam 5m: Em: mo o.oo 9 3 9wm _.oou 93A :39 9mm 9 _ m 99 930 98— _.amw >338 Kong 09% -98 9mm 93 9m; 9.:— _.SA 93 9N0 o.oo _.SN 9%: r3; 9’: -98 98 93 903 9a.: _.od -93 93 _._w 993 93m fog >mo 93 93 95 _.omq 93o _.Amw 9cm 9: 9S _.oB 93m _.wfl zwc Can 98 93 3.3.1. _.owm _.Sa fog 98 92 .98... _.Sm _.oom _.omm 58393: gono— Umrw -93 93 93 9.39 9qu _.mmm -98 98 9S 9an 9H3 _.qfi EE -93 98 93 90am 93o _.oqm -93 98 9% o.ofl 9me _.owm >ma 9g 9 fl m 9 H w _bS 9.3M _.Amo 9cm 9 3 9 E _.oma 939 _.wom ch C8 98 93 5.3.... _.SN _.Sm _.oao 98 93 98: _.S o. _.ooo _.owm Um?” x ch Gun -92 92 93 obs 903 _.So -98 93 .35... 903 903 o.ooo g A .3. ”So-8:3. .2. h A .3. 35-8w _na. 197 odds ratio of .983 (95% CI = .967, .999) for every unit increase in the interaction term (see Figure 4, p. 62, for illustration of the interaction effect). Difierentiation & Perception of Best Friend at MS U '5 Alcohol Use Males. The Additive Model with perceptions of their best friend at MSU’s alcohol consumption was significant overall (38(4) = 70.40, p < .001, R20”, & Sm" = .44, Rznme = .58) with a significant block improvement over the Basic Model (x20) = 68.19, p < .001), and had successful prediction rate of 84.6%, an increase 29.3% over the Basic model. The only significant individual predictor in the model was perception of best friend use, which illustrated a difference of 1 unit of perception made it 1.04 times more likely the subject was a High versus a Low Drinker (see Table 48, p. 199). A difference in perceptions by a value of one standard deviation (72.43) improved the odds of being classified as a high drinker by 13.56 times. The Interaction Model also was significant overall (38(5) = 72.77, p < .001, R20,,( & 3...... = .45, [fink-fining“,e = .60) and had a successful prediction rate of 84.65%; however, it did not show a block improvement over the Additive Model (x20) = 2.37, p = .124). Again, the only significant individual variable was perceptions of best friend use which remained similar to the Additive Model (OR = 1.041, 95% CI = 1.026, 1.056, see Table 48, p. 199). 198 flea—o Am rommmno Wow—.038: 39:35 930:8 Em: Em: <29; r02 013.83 I hen—.8303 cm 88. 353% CR _ 3&8 _ deE8 9 e _ mm _ as: _ ow _ 8.x. 9 _ s _ mm _ sea _ ow _ 8.x. 9 mean zona— Ume -98 98 5mm 99..» 98.8 whom -98 98 93 993 93¢ #88 El: o.oo 98 9S foo.» 98o fog -98 98 _.8 980 98c _.omA >mo 93 9: 98 room 93A _.fio 98 93 92 996 98; _.88 >853 Zena Um?” -98 93 93 98o 9.33 Mb; 9; 98m 98 :8 9%: Pmmm BE -98 93 93 903 980 _.oow -98 98 9N9 930 98w. _.o3 >mo o.oo 9E o.oo o.ooo 93m _.mo_ -93 9 _ m 93 98c 98m _.88 mom" 333% can 93 93 89.3: #83 _.oNA 28¢ 98 93 No.51. _.oka _.omc _.o8 58330: Zona— Umrw 9mm 93 9B EMA”... 953 w. _ m; -9: 93 93 98m 98o _.mao W72 -98 93 98; 908 980 :3 -98 98 98 93c 98o room >mo 98 9B 98 _.8m 9me _.mmu -93 9 _ o 98 903 933 _.wmm wow" man-baa :8 98 93 892 _._... foe: _.owa bag 98 93 8.8.1. _.OAo _.8_ foam UmH-W X wnamnosaa 96 98 93 N8 #33 98m #95 -98 93 8.3 903 908 _.o8 _. w A .8. 30.823. _._... h A .2. 25.3209 Females. The Additive Model with perceptions of their best friend at MSU’s alcohol consumption was significant overall (352(4) = 55.99, p < .001, RIC“ & 5m" = .31, 1?sz = .42) with a significant block improvement over the Basic Model (780) = 53.93, p < .001), and had successful prediction rate of 80.7%, an increase 28% over the Basic model. Perceptions of best friend’s alcohol use significantly changed the odds of being a High versus Low Drinker by 1.046 (95% CI = 1.029, 1.063) times for a one unit change in perception (see Table 48, p. 199) and 6.47 times greater for a change equivalent to one standard deviation change (41.48). The Interaction Model also was significant overall (38(5) = 59.09, p < .001, RZCO,‘ & gm" = .33, Rznmnmke = .43) and had a successful prediction rate of 79.3%, but did not show a block improvement over the Additive Model (352(1) = 3.11, p = .078). The perception of best friend use remained the only significant individual predictor with the values remaining nearly identical (OR = 1.049, 95% CI = 1.031, 1.068, see Table 48, p. 199). Differentiation & C 0A Status Males. The Additive Model with COA status failed to reach a level of significance (12(4) = 3.20, p = .525, see Table 49, p. 201). The Interaction Model also failed to reach significance (38(5) = 3.29, p = .655). Females. The Additive Model with COA status failed to reach a level of significance (78(4) = 2.09, p = .719, see Table 49, p. 201). The Interaction Model also was significant overall (38(5) = 4.959, p = .421). 200 .930 he rommmmo ”3338: 32:33 mama-ha 9% 20 En: 535 hog 01383 I 00> wSEm _ 3&8 h moaaom _s _mm _ fiaa _ on: 839 _ a _mmnéa: 9: 8:9 momma Zona— UmH-W -9Aw 9.3 :3 9?; 9w? _.Nom .98 93 93 993 93o _.Acw wz: o.oo 93 93 focus 930 _.ccm -9o~ 93 _.mu 980 o.ooo _.oaA >mo o.oo 9: 9mm _.oow 93A :39 98- 9G 93 933 903 _.mmu >355" 383 Uwzw -93. 93 _.3 93m 93— _.NE -93 9Nm 90A 993 9m: 3.3x E’s: 93 93 98 _.oom 908 _.ccc -9om 9cm _.3 9vo 93V memo >mo 9cm 9: 93 .93 9x: _.Awa -93 93 93 98» 9.3m when 00> -9? 9A0 93 930 9N5 _.moo 93 93 9E _.omw 93o Nara 58330: Zeal UwH-W -9“; 9.3 _.qm 93o 9mg rumm .93 9mm 9mm 9.3M 93m _.umN W73 93 93 98 _.ooo 98M _.oco -98 93 _.mm 98m 93m _.owm >mn 93 93 93 _.oww 93w TEN -93 9: 93 98c ohmu _.Noq 00> .9A0 93 _.ow 93w 9N3 rum: o.oo 93 98 HbOu 93— ~08 UMH-WX 00> 98. 9a o.oo _.Noo 9~mw whoa f; 9.3 Now PM: 93m Sham a. so A .3. 25-8209 3.. h A .3. Eco-EFF 201 Difierentiation & Expectations of A lcohol Serving as a Tension Reducer Males. The Additive Model with expectations of alcohol as a tension reducer was significant overall (12(4) = 27.64, p < .001, RICO, & 3....- = .20, RzNwm -—- .27) with a significant block improvement over the Basic Model (x20) = 25.43, p < .001), and had successful prediction rate of 67.5%, an increase 12.2% over the Basic model. The tension reduction variable was significant for subject’s in that they were 1.137 (95% CI = 1.075, 1.203) times more likely to be a High versus a Low Drinker with a 1 unit difference in tension reduction expectations (see Table 50, p. 203). Subjects with one standard deviation (8.73) higher expectations are 3.08 times more likely to be High Drinkers. The Interaction Model also was significant overall (36(5) = 27.64, p < .001, RZCO, & 5m" = .20, Rznmm, = .27), had a successful prediction rate of 67.5%, but did not significantly show a block improvement over the Additive Model (12(1) = .00, p = .993). The only significant individual variable in the model, expectations of tension reduction, maintained the same values as the Additive Model (OR = 1.137, 95% CI = 1.075, 1.203, see Table 50, p. 203). Females. The Additive Model with expectations of alcohol as a tension reducer was significant overall (78(4) = 35.99, p < .001, we,” 5...“ = .21, R’Nm-m = .28) with a significant block improvement over the Basic Model (780) = 33.93, p < .001). The model had a successful prediction rate of 69.3%, an increase 16.6% over the Basic model. The tension reduction variable was a significant individual predictor for the increased odds of being classified as a High versus Low Drinker (OR = 1.146, 95% CI = 1.087, 1.209) for a one unit change in tension reduction (see Table 50, p. 203) and (OR = 3.31) for a change equivalent to one standard deviation (8.81). 202 Hugo mo rommmno Womwommmo: 32:38 930:8 92 Em: 325 has Gianna I mxvoosmoam om H255: Woacnaos _ 3&8 _ moan—om _ a _ mm _ as: _ ow # 8.x. 9 _ a 4 mm _ fies _ ow _ 3.x. 9 wmmmo zona— Uwrw -98 98 #8 98¢ 98m ~88 -98 98 98 90.3 980 #38 wz: o.oo 98 9S foo; 98c #08 -98 98 _.8 980 o.ooo _.oan >mn 98 93 98 _.o8 98A :13 98 98 92 926 993 #88 >335 381 Um?” -98 93 98 998 930 No8 98 98 _._o _.80 9.3a 9qu 95 98 98 98. _.8A 988 :8 -98 98 93 98c 98o _.oum >mo 93 93 98 :8 98¢ H6: 98 93 98 r8_ 98c 88 Adamo: ”8:38 98 98 898...... :3 _.o-G ~88 93 98 8.8.3.. :3 _.ow-N _.Noc 58830: Keno— Umzw -98 93 o.oo 98A 9A8 8.8m 98 98 :8 _.Sq 9.3a ~un 95 98 98 9B 58; 988 _.88 -98 98 93 98o 98a roux >mo 93 95 98 :8 980 _.m: 98 93 98 _.88 9§o ~88 H088: Waggon 98 98 3.3.... :3 _.o-G _.88 9E 98 8.8.... :3 _.omu r88 Um?” X H958: Waggon o.oo 98 98 race 90: roe-N 98 98 92 903 903 _.8A _._ h A .8. "so-8:09 sale A .o . go-§_oa. 203 The Interaction Model also was significant overall (£6) = 36.00, p < .001, R20“ & 5...," = .21, [CZ-imam = .28) and had a successful prediction rate of 70%, but did not show a significant block improvement over the Additive Model (12(1) = .01, p = .93 8). The tension reduction variable remained the only a significant individual predictor for the increased odds of being classified as a High versus Low Drinker (OR = 1.146, 95% CI = 1.087, 1.209, see Table 50, p. 203). Ditfirentiation & Expectations of Alcohol Serving as a Social Lubricant Males. The Additive Model with expectations of alcohol as a social lubricant was significant (38(4) = 15.83, p = .003, RZCOX-g 5...," = .12, Rzuw = .16) with a significant block improvement over the Basic Model (x2(1)= 13.63, p < .001) and had a successful prediction rate of 67 5%, an increase 12.2% over the Basic model. The social lubrication variable was the only significant individual variable that changed the odds of drinking classification. Having a social lubrication value change by 1 unit increased the likelihood ofbeing a High Drinker by 1.114 (95% CI = 1.047, 1.184) times (see Table 51, p. 205). Additionally, a difference in social lubrication scores equivalent to one standard deviation (7.89) improved the odds of being a high drinker by 2.35. The Interaction Model also was significant overall (12(5) = 15.84, p = .007, RICO,- & Sm" = .12, Rznm = .16) and had a successful prediction rate of 66.7%; however, it did not show a block improvement over the Additive Model (x20) = .01, p = .923). The social lubricant variable maintained the same level of influence 1.114 (95% CI = 1.047, 1.185) as the Additive Model (see Table 51, p. 205). 204 .320 m. rommmno ”3333: 32:35 $836 9:» Em: «688 res: 0138?. I 9685303 om meow”: 95183:: 7 73:8 _ mafia—om _ 8 _mm _ 9:»: _ o: _ 8:9 _ :c _ mm_ 9:»: _ o: _ 39.9 awmmo Kean— :m; -93 93 _.3 9%: 9:: 9:8 -98 9:: 9% 9:3 93: :8 :7: 98 98 9S :9: 9:8 988 -98 9i 2: 98: 9:8 5% >9” 9% 9:. 9% S8 93.. 9.3: 9:: 9a 92 923 98: 93: >maa o 9; 93 SN :3 9:: 98m 92 9a 98 :5 9:; IE moaaEEsaS 9: 98 :3: I: :3: 9:: 9: 98 3:... :8 SS _._: 583030: Zeno— :m; -98 9.3 98 9B: 93” 28 9.: 9:: Z: 38 9:9. 5% :3: 98 98 9:. 2:: 9S: 9:: -9o: 98 5o 98: 98. 2.: 2a 9; 9a _.3 :3 92: 9%: 98 9: 98 993 9:: 98: mos». €388: 9: 98 :8”: Z: 2:: :3 9: 98 3.2: 9:: 22 9:: SEQ mega—.3188: 92 98 92 28 9:8 :8 98 99: 98 98: 9:; 993 _._-u A .8. arc-S89 3...: A .2. «So-8:69 205 Females. The Additive Model with expectations of alcohol as a social lubricant was significant overall (36(4) = 22.34, p < .001, RICO, & 5,... = .14, RIM“, = .19) with a significant block improvement over the Basic Model (380) = 20.29, p < .001). The model had a successful prediction rate of 67.3%, an increase 14.6% over the Basic model. Only social lubrication was a significant individual predictor for High versus Low Drinker, with individuals having a 1 unit change being 1.120 (95% CI = 1.061, 1.181) times more likely to be a High Drinker (see Table 51, p. 205), and 2.40 times more likely for a difference equivalent to one standard deviation (7.74). The Interaction Model also was significant overall (x26) = 22.35, p < .001, chm a 3...," = .14, Rzumc = .19) and had a successful prediction rate of 67.3%; however, it did not show a block improvement over the Additive Model (x20) = .00, p = .950). Social lubrication remained the only significant individual predictor for high versus low drinkers, with individuals having a1 unit change being 1.119 (95% CI=1.061,1.181) times more likely to classified as a high drinker (see Table 51, p. 205). The Comprehensive Model Males. The Comprehensive Model was significant according to the model chi- square (38(13) = 86.95, p < .001, Ric... & 3...... = .51, RZNW. = .68) and showed a significant improvement over the Basic Model (x200) = 45.97, p < .001). The model illustrated an 87% success rate in predicting the correct drinking classifications, an improvement of 3 1 .7% over the Basic Model. Perception of best fi'iend usage improved the likelihood of being classified as a high drinker (OR = 1.037, 95% CI = 1.021, 1.053) while being a COA actually decreased the likelihood of being a high drinker versus low drinker (OR = .188, 95% CI = .037, .947, see Table 52, p. 207). 206 .8:—o 8 :ommmmo womammmoz 3.38:8 maaoam 9m. Em: 588.. :02 Una—83 I 0950883?" Zeno— ?88 8388 8 mm ’58 OW 88 Q 8 mm «<95 0w 8.8 Q Um?” -98 9mm 98 90:o 9w _ N 8.80 93 98 9 _ m :88 988 98: 9,3 -98 98 98 908 980 :Go -98 98 9 8 98: 980 :88 >8 98 98 98 :8o 98: ::8 9o: 98 9 _ N :o:~ 9:8: :88 ch 09.80308 98 92 mm: :8N 908 :8. 98 93 888 :28 :o8 :80 CE.” x 3m: 82.8083 -93 98 98 908 908 :80 -98 93 N8 908 98c :28 wow. m 125 98 93 89.3... _._ :8: :8_ :08 98 93 ~98: :8m :8: :80 02% x wow» 3.8.5 98 9S :8 :3m 900a :88 -98 98 8.88... 90:0 908 900m 00> :8: 98 8.5... 9:8 98: 98: 9:0 980 95 98: 98c N80 Umvw X 00> :8 :8 90m 883 98.8 ::.:8 98 98 98 :80 9N0o _:m8 883: ”3:08: 93 98 N8 :8 908 :88 98 98 _98: :8 :8~ :88 Ume x 8:80: ”8:30: -92 93 98 908 93c :Noo -98 98 98 908 98: :8 meow».— ::c:omao= -98 98 98 908 988 :_ _o 98 98 9N0 :o8 908 :— ; Um?” x moan. reggae: 98 98 98 :88 938 :N0_ 98 98 9.0 :o8 988 :83 ca A .8. 23-889 _._... .a A .2. 2.98:8. 207 Females. The Comprehensive Model was significant (3803) = 88.84, p < .001, RZCO,‘ & gm“ = .45, Rszlmkc = .60) and showed a significant improvement over the Basic Model (x2(10)= 86.81, p < .001). The model illustrated an 84.7% success rate in predicting the correct drinking classifications, an improvement of 32% over the Basic Model. Perceptions of MSU student use (OR = 1.016, 95% CI = 1.002, 1.029). perceptions of best fiiend use (OR = 1.048, 95% CI = 1.027, 1.069), the interaction between differentiation and the perception of best fi'iend use (OR = .970, 95% CI = .942, .998), and tension reduction (OR = 1.135, 95% CI = 1.052, 1.225) (see Table 52, p. 207). High Drinkers versus Moderate Drinkers Differentiation and GA U Males. The Basic Model failed to reach a level of significance as an overall model (36(3) = 5.851 p = .119, see Table 53, p. 209). Females. The Basic Model failed to reach a level of significance as an overall model (78(3) = 4.095, p = .251, see Table 53, p. 209). F our factors of the DSI-R with GA U Males. The four-factor model was significant as an overall model (78(6) = 16.35, p = .012, chmg Smu= .13, Rznwm = .17) and had a 61.5% success rate in predicting the correct drinking classifications, an improvement of 5.8% over the Basic Mode]. Interestingly, a 1 unit increase in Emotional Reactivity (being less emotionally reactive) increased the likelihood of being a High Drinker (OR = 1.928, 95% CI = .999, 3.720), whereas increasing the “I” Position value by 1 unit decreased the likelihood of being a High versus a Moderate Drinker (OR = .374, 95% CI = .198, .706, see Table 54, p. 209). 208 4min mm rommmmo womammmo: wammonam macaoam EB Em: 535.. 2.3230 Una—22m I Gama—ammo: 3&8 man—£8 w mm 9&5 0w 3.x. 9 w mm €95 OW 8.x. 9 Um?” -93 93 9S 9§¢ 930 Home 93 93 98 _._oo 93w Poem Eé 93 93 who _._.3 93A _.Nou -93 o.oo _.qn 995 993 _.owm >mo 93 93 9mm :3 93a _.mua -98 93 Num 9.3a 9mg room _._ h A .8. 35-3%? .2. E A .3. 23-6%? HmEo m; hommmmo ~398me vanaaasm mannaa Em: Em: 585 33968 Una—88 I moE $883 w 2 3&8 moafiom v mm 9:3 or 3.x. 9 w mm €95 OW 8.x. 9 magmas»— WomoaiQ 99w 9m» 93... #on 980 VHS 95 9mm 98 _._Na 9mg _.mNm .4: $830: -93 9mm 93.... 93A 93% 93a -98 9X 98 9vo 93» _.m8 magmas». 050m. 98 93 93 _.NB 93o Nb; 9: 98 92 Z 3 .23 :2» Tie: 2:: 092m 9% 93 _.3 93c 98. :3 -93 9.3 93 9me 93m _.uom EE 9 _ w 93 P: :3 99mm _.won -93 98 _.2 98m 93m #93 >mo o.oo 93 9w» fog 93m _Lom -98. 9; Nu— 9q3 9mg _.oam .. h A bu. 35-8%? .2. V A .3. 25-8%? Females. As was the case in the Basic Model, the four-factor model failed to reach a level of significance as an overall model (352(6) = 4.49, p = .61, see Table 54, p. 209). Dmerentiation & perceptions of the average MSU student ’s alcohol use Males. The Additive Model with perceptions of their best friend at MSU’s alcohol consumption failed to reach a level of significance (12(4) = 8.455, p = .076, see Table 55, p. 212). However, the Interaction Model was significant overall (386) = 13.21, p = .022, RICO, & 5...,“ = . 10, Rznmmm = .14) and had a successful prediction rate of 60.7%,‘and showed a significant a block improvement over the Basic Model (38(2) = 7.36, p = .03). The interaction of differentiation and perception of MSU student use was a significant individual predictor with individuals being .978 (95% CI = .957, .999) times as likely to be a High versus a Moderate Drinker (see Table 55, p. 212, and see Figure 3, p. 60, for illustration of the interaction efl'ect). Females. The Additive Model with perceptions of MSU student alcohol consumption failed to reach significance as an overall model (x2(4) = 9.05, p = .060, see Table 55, p. 212). However, the Interaction Model was significant overall (13(5) = 11.52, p = .042, R29” 3nd. = .08, Rzrmeumke = .11) and had a successful prediction rate of 65.0%, however, it did not show a block improvement over the Basic Model (38(2) = 7.43, p = .024). Although it was not significant, the interaction between differentiation and perception of MSU student use removed the significance of MSU student use as an individual variable (see Table 55, p. 212). 210 Dijferentiation & Perception of Best Friend at MS U ’s Alcohol Use Males. The Additive Model with perceptions of their best friend at MSU’s alcohol consumption was significant overall (12(4) = 55.99, p =< .000, R29“ & gm“ = .31, from“: = .42) with a significant block improvement over the Basic Model (380) = 35.81, p = .000), and had successful prediction rate of 76.2%, an increase 20.5% over the Basic model. The perception of best friend use increased the likelihood of being a High versus a Moderate Drinker by 1.024 (95% CI = 1.013, 1.034, see Table 56, p. 214) times for a one unit change in perception and 5.29 times for a difference equivalent of one standard deviation (72.43). The Interaction Model also was significant overall (76(5) = 59.09, p < .001, k’m & 3.," = .33, Rzumu‘m = .43) and had a successful prediction rate of 76.2%, but did not show a block improvement over the Additive Model (360) = .45, p = .504). Perception of best fiiend use remained the only significant independent variable in the categorization of High versus Moderate Drinkers and remained equivalent to the values in the Additive Model (OR = 1.023, 95% CI = 1.013, 1.033, see Table 56, p. 214). Females. The Additive Model with perceptions of their best friend at MSU’s alcohol consumption was significant (78(4) = 12.05, p = .017, R20», & s..." = .08, Rznwm = .11) with a significant block improvement over the Basic Model (x20) = 7.96, p = .005). The model had a successful prediction rate of 61 .3%, an increase 2.2% over the Basic model. Perception of best friend use was a significant individual variable with individuals that had a one unit higher value on best friend perception were 1.012 (95% CI = 1.003, 1.021) times more likely to be a Higher versus a Moderate Drinker (see Table 56, p. 214). The change in odds for being a high drinker increased to 1.65 times 211 H.920 mm rommmao Womaommmos 333:5 mannam 98 Em: % 9: 9; Sn :3 Sam 33 -98 9; N3 o.ooo o.ooo 2% >338 gono— Umm-W -9~m 9g 93 9am 9ch rm-R 95 9: 93 _.N_N 93m Nun—o Ext: 9: 93 NS _.Sn 90.3 _.N-S -93 9ko _.3 98¢ 98M #03 >Mo 95 93 9am _._wc 930 _.MB -95 93 _.Am 93o 93m _._3 ZmCCmo 93 93 New :5» 908 _.o: 93 93 PS... 53o _.oS _.owo 383020: Zona— Uwzw -98 93 9: 93m 93m ~68 9mm 9: 93 _.NAM 93a Puma wz: 9: 93 me 92a 9on _.wa -98 9cm _.2 995 93m _.oma >mo 9B 93 9mm :2 9qu _.mwm -93 93 _.E 9mAc 93m _.Iq chcmo 93 9o— TS _.coa 90cm _.03 9o— 92 98 _.So _.ooo _.Sc Umzwx ch can -98 92 Puma 93w ohm-N 908 -98 93 who 90% chow _.oom a A .3. 35.823. _._... .u A .S. "So-8:8. 212 more likely when the difference between perception scores was equal to one standard deviation (41.48). The Interaction Model also was significant overall (x2(5) = 12.45, p = .029, RZCO,‘ & 5...," = .09, [ills-ageing, = .12) and bad a successful prediction rate of 61 .3%, but did not show a block improvement over the Additive Model (12(1) = .40, p = .527). Perception of best fi'iend use remained the only significant individual variable in changing the likelihood of being a High versus Moderate Drinker (OR =1 .013, 95% CI = 1.003, 1.021, see Table 56, p. 214). Differentiation & COA Status Males. The Additive Model with COA status failed to reach a level of significance (78(4) = 6.98, p = .137, see Table 57, p. 215). The Interaction Model also failed to reach a level significance (12(5) = 7.55, p = .183). Females. The Additive Model with COA status failed to reach a level of significance as an overall model (38(4) = 4.10, p = .393, see Table 57, p. 215). The Interaction Model also failed to reach a level of significance (x26) = 6.63, p = .249). Dijferentiation & Expectations of Alcohol Serving as a Tension Reducer Males. The Additive Model with expectations of alcohol as a tension reducer failed to reach a level of significance (38(4) = 8.77, p = .067, see Table 58, p. 216). Females. The Additive Model with expectations of alcohol as a tension reducer failed to reach significance (38(4) = 7.34, p = .119, see Table 58, p. 216). The interaction Model also failed to achieve a level of significance (38(5) = 8.202, p = .145). 213 flea—a 3 rommman womammmoa 333:5 wagons =5” ”8 an: 388 33038 01383 I $28303 3- W8" magma Cwo _ 3&8 _ moan—Em _ a _mm_ are: _09_ 8x9 _ a _mm_ fin: _ 2: 39.9 wmmmo Zona— cm; -98 93 93 9s; 8% 38 98 98 98 :8 9m: No8 my: 9: 98 use 2w... 98a 58 -93 98 SN 98o 98m _.8... >ma 9: 9; 93 :3 935 _.aa -98 9; 93 93a 9M8 23 >a&mmo o.oo 93 9M... _.ocw 93m #3; -93 93 who 93o 93mm _.33 womfimaosadcma 98 92 3.3.... _.ONA _.Su row.“ 93 o.oo 93...... _.SN _.oow rom— EfiBoaos Zona— szw -93 93 93 93m 930 ~33 93 93 93 _._-3 9m-NN 9A3 wzm 93 98 93 _.3o 93w _.wAc -93 93 _.3 93m 930 _.95 >mb. 90¢ 93 9mm _.ocw 93M _.mow -93 93 N3 9.2: 933 _.omm wamfimloamw :8 98 9o— Nouo: fiowu _.Sw _.Owu 93 93 Soon... fo—w _.oow _.omw 02% X wow» magma C3 -93 93 93 908 9on _.oE 93 9S 9% foom 90cc _.owo _._-u A .3. "so-$39 1.9 A .2. 3.0-4.359 214 .320 mu Famine Womammmo: 33835 magnum 92 Ed Em: 385 gonna—8 U118; I 00> mSEm _ 3&8 _ 122:8 _ u _ mm _ $5 3 oz _ 8.x. 9 3 w _ mm 75:: oz 9 8.x. 9 P50 zona— Ume .93 93 93 9:6 93o _.mmo 93 9mm 98 :8 93a Nova 93 9 _ u 93 who _._: 903 _.Now -93 93 _.qw 98o 93m :35 >mo 93 93 93 :3 93m _.mwm -98 93 Mom 9qu 93m _.oao >aa§mo 9G 93 9mm :3 9%?“ _.mm_ -98 93 93 9qu 9mg :33 00> -93 93 _._N 9mg 98o 9mm». -98 93 93 908 93c Nuum 38330: 380. 03.” -93 93 _.ou 93a 93.‘ rmom -9 E 93 9 E 93; 93w 9.3m EE 9 3 A 93 Brow“. :3 _.OOA 3b _ m -93 93 _.oo 9on 93m _.Ouw >mo 9G 9G 93 :3 93¢ rum”. -9? 93 who 9.23 9qu _.ome 00> -9mm 93 _.wo 9&0 9B~ 9A: -93 9g o.oo 93o 9min N»; 03.9 x 00> 93 93 9% 3b: 93; _9w3 :6 98 Pun A.A”: 93m w9m3 _.. .u A .3, 25-8209 1% A .3 . 25-559 215 HmEo mm rommmzo Womaommmo: $9538 95an 92 98 En: 835 30838 013.88 I mxvaofiaoam om Hoswmoa flamenco: _ 3&8 _ mesa—om _ v _ mm 9 a»: 9 ow _ 8.x. 9 _ w _ mm _ 55 _ ox _ 8.x. 9 wmmwo Zona— Umvw -98 93 98 .Eo 98¢ 83 93 98 98 :8 983 No8 EE 9 8 w 93 who _._“: 903 _.N8 -93 98 _.qm 98o 988 58a >mo 9: 9G 98 :3 98a _.8m -98 93 N8 93¢ 988 fog >833 Zona— Uwvw -98 93 98 905 920 905 98 9mm 98 88 9mm: 9me EE 9 _ m 98 8.8 2.: 993 83 -98 98 _.uo 98m 980 _.ofi >ma 9 H m 9 _ 8 _.AN _.Nom 98m _.mwm -98 9 _ m 9.3 93m 9ch rot Anzac: won—8:0: 98 98 NM: rot 98w :8 98 98 8.: r8_ 98A _._ I 58828: 38¢. 62% -93 9t 98 908 92: 9on 9B 98m 93 _.N8 988 N58 wz: 9 B 98. 8.2 :2 98a 88 -98 98 _.8 98a 98o _.8m >mo 9_ m 9 _ a 8g :8 988 ram; -98 9 _ o 8.8 98¢ 98m “.88 Adamo: ”8:30: 98 98 N? _.onm 98w :8 98 98 8.8 #8; 9o3 _._ _m Ume x Anzac: romance: 92 98 98 _.oom 98m :8 98 98 98 reg 98c :8 _._fi A .8u 25-889 _._... u A .3. 25.823. 216 Difierentiation & Expectations of Alcohol Serving as a Social Lubricant Males. The Additive Model with expectations of alcohol as a tension reducer failed to reach a level of significance (38(4) = 6.933, p = .139, see Table 59, p. 218). The Interaction Model also failed to reach a level of significance (38(5) = 6.939, p = .225). Females. The Additive Model with expectations of alcohol as a social lubricant failed to reach significance (36(4) = 5.45, p = .244, see Table 59, p. 218). The Interaction Model also failed to achieve a level of significance (76(5) = 5.58, p = .350). The Comprehensive Model Males. The Comprehensive Model was significant according to the model chi- square (x203) = 51 .44, p < .001, RICO, a s..." = .34, Rimm, = .46) and showed a significant improvement over the Basic Model (x2(10)= 45.59, p < .001). The model illustrated a 74.6% success rate in predicting the correct drinking classifications, an improvement of 18.9% over the Basic Model. Perception of best friend use was the only significant individual variable in the Comprehensive Model (OR = 1.026, 95% CI 1.014, 1.038, see Table 60, p. 219). Females. The Comprehensive Model was significant according to the model chi- square (x203) = 23.41, p = 037,189,... 5,... = .16, mm... = .21) and showed a significant improvement over the Basic Model (x2(10)= 19.32, p = .04). The model illustrated a 65% success rate in predicting the correct drinking classifications, an improvement of 5.9% over the Basic Model. Interestingly, taken as a whole the model was significant, however, no individual predictors in this model significantly changed the odds of being a High Drinker versus a Moderate Drinker (see Table 60, p. 219). 217 .320 we rommmzo ”$388: 38338 mange 93 m3 Em: 58% 38088 01388 I mxuooSaoa om moor: raw—mouse: fl 3&8 _ moBEom _ a _ mm _ 2o; _ o: _ 8:9 _ a _ mm H ,5: o: _ 8.x. 9 wmmmo Zena 0mm-” -93 93 9¢N 938 98c _.8o 95 98. 98 :8 93.3 No8 ES— 98 93 who _._: 908 _.N8 -93 98 _.3N 98o 9m8 _.8¢ >8 9: 98 98 :3 98¢ _.8¢ -98 98 N8 93¢ 988 _.8¢ >835. 333 Ume -9 fl 8 9.8 9 I 98c 98; _.30 9N¢ 98 98 _.Noo 9¢s5 ~.¢~¢ m3: 9 _ A 93 98¢... _.TK moon _.83 -98 98 #8 98m 9mg _.oao >mo 98 98 :A _.3¢ 938 .88 -98 98 NE 98; 983 _.33 moan— rccnowmo: 98 98 _.3 _.8_ 938 room 98 98 _.8 _.8m 93¢ _.08 58:83:: :83 Um?” .93 93 98 98¢ 98m #80 98 98¢ 9mm #83 9¢3 ~88 EE 9 I 93 u .93... _._: _.oow _.w3 -98 98 93 9ko 9?: :8“ >8. 9 _ ¢ 9_ u _.E _.3¢ 938 ~88 -93 9 _ m _ .3 98¢ 9¢8 _.omm mooi r8183? 98 98 _ .3 _.8w 93m _.ooA 98 98 _.3 #8; 93¢ 58¢ 09-x X moan. C5188: 98 98 9S _.ooa 908 :3 -98 98 98 908 98¢ _.omo am A .8. 25-889 .3. .: A .3. 25-889 218 Ania ¢o homage wamaammmo: 0898988 850:8 9m» m8 Em: 885 80888 03:88 I 0030330830 Zona— Km—om moan—am 8 mm «$8 CW 88 Q 8 mm 8:5 CW 88 Q 02% -93 980 _.03 98 9 EN ..3¢ 9 _ m 98 9 3 :8 98.. 8.88 2,5 9 _ ¢ 930 N8 :8 908 _.3¢ -98 98 98 903 98— ~38 >ma 98 98 9 _ 8 .38 988 _.¢- -98 9 _ 8 _.8 980m 988 _.38 Km: 88388 98 93 98 9000 908 _.SN 92 9S _.8 _.38 908 .38 Umzw X 78: 880083 -98 93 8.8 98¢ 90% .38 -98 9S _.8 908 983 ~38 mam. manna 98 93 8.83.. :08 _.o _ A _.38 92 92 P8 #80 9000 _.83 08% X mam. $82. -92 93 98 900m 908 ~38 93 92 98 _.38 908 ~38 00> L . _ ¢ 98 8.3 9w _ w 930. _.8¢ -98 9mm 98 9un 98A No: Umzw X 00> _.8 r8 _.8 .988 988 8.33 #8 _.30 _.8 ”80¢ 93¢ 8988 838: 8988: 98 98 93 _.88 98¢ :3 98 98 Nun _.88 908 _._”: 02-x X 838: 8908: 9: 9: 90o : 8 980. .80.» 98 98 _.8 _.8_ 908 #85 moan— rcgomao: -98 98 90¢ 908 98m _.88 98 98 98 _.8_ 903 :08 Um?” X momma 55188: -98 98 98 90.: 98¢ 83.» -98 98 98 908 98.. _.08 _._ h A .8. 25-328. .2. h A .3 . 23-889 219 Appendix M Analyses of Binge Drinking Use As was done with the General Alcohol Use measure, the continuous Binge Drinking variable (number of days in the past 14 days the subject drank at binge levels - - 5 drinks per occasion for males, 4 drinks per occasion for females) was converted into a categorical variable consisting of 4 levels of binge drinking: Abstainers (those that did not drink), Nonbinging Drinkers (subjects that consumed alcohol, but not at binge levels), Occasional Binge Drinkers (1 or 2 times in the past 2 weeks), and Frequent Binge Drinkers (3 or more times in the past 2 weeks) (see Appendix J). As the four categories reflect increasing degrees of alcohol consumption, the nature of this study’s hypotheses which was stated for a continuous dependent variable (e. g., students lower levels of differentiation will be more likely to have greater amounts of binge drinking) was maintained with the categorical analyses (e.g., students lower levels of differentiation will be more likely to have a greater relationship with Frequent Binge Drinkers than Occasional Binge Drinkers when controlling for BMI and age). The four binge drinking categories were contrasted in ways such that four unique outcome dichotomies were created: H . Binge Drinkers (Occasional & Frequent Bingers) versus Nonbinging Drinkers 2. Occasional Binge Drinkers versus Nonbinging Drinkers E” Frequent Binge Drinkers versus Nonbinging Binge Drinkers 4. Frequent Binge Drinkers versus Occasional Binge Drinkers The same coding procedure fiom the General Alcohol Use variables was implemented with the Binge Drinking variables with the greater prevalence of binging was coded as a 220 “1” in SPSS and the lesser category was coded as a “O.” For example, in the Bingers to Nonbinging Drinkers analyses, all persons in the Occasional and Frequent Binging categories were coded as a “1” and Nonbinging Drinkers were coded as a “O.” Bingers (Occasional & Frequent Bingers) versus Nonbingers DWrentiation and binge drinking Males. The Basic Model was not significant according to the model chi-square (38(3) = .75, p = .861, see Table 61, p. 222). Females. The Basic Model failed to reach a level of significance according to the model chi-square_(x2(3) = 3.38, p = .337, see Table 61, p. 222). F our- factors of the DSI-R (Emotional Reactivity, “1” Position, Emotional Cutofl,‘ and Fusion with Others) with binge drinking Males. As was the case in the Basic Model, the overall four-factor model failed to reach a level of significance 08(6) = 1.327, p = .970, see Table 62, p. 222). Females. As was the case in the Basic Model, the overall four-'factor model failed to reach a level of significance (x2(6) = 4.987, p = .546, see Table 62, p. 222). Di/firentiation & perception of average MSU student ’s alcohol use Males. The Additive Model with perceptions of the average MSU student’s alcohol consumption was not significant as an overall model (36(4) = 5.616, p = .230, see Table 63, p. 223). The Interaction Model also was not significant as an overall model (38(5) = 5.923, p = .314). 221 Hugo 3 rommmmo Nowaommmoa 32:35 manna—a 93 93 Ooommmoaa gamma <22; 2039508 I 059.3323: 25.8 mafia—om w mm $33 OW 3.x. 9 w mm «Sm:— Ow 8.x. 0— Umzw -98 93 93 9on 93m H28 .98 93. 93 93m 93m #me 9’: -98 93 9B 993 9mm~ _.omw -93 93 New 9ch 930 fiooo >mo 9B 93 98 _._: 93m _.mmo 9cm 9; 9; _.omm 93m :30 _._ .m A .8. go-8=om. .15 A .2. "So-Smog. Hug—o am rommmao womaommmoa mammogm 9:936 92 3a wmamaa mo 93 93 9% _._-3 93m _.mam 93 93 9mm _.omo 930 _.wma _._ E A .3. "So-€39 I. k A .2 . Hie-8:8. 222 H920 3 homage ”£8.88: 32:35 mama-=8 53 98 gamma «685 Zoacmswoa I 383303 om zwc manna Cmn T :83 _ maamfim _ N. Hmm A 2% _ 2: 882 _ 8 _mm :59: 9: 889 mum? Zona— cm; :98 98 98 988 988 SS -98 93 98 9% 938 988 $2 -98 98 9B 98. 98M :5 -93 98 98 903 98c 28 >9. 9: 9a 98 _._: 938 38 98 9; 9; 988 938 3.3 >ma3mn 9B 9: 9mm EBA 9m; _.m-Nm o.oo 9; ohm room 9mm~ _.waq ch Cmo 98 9S PmA _.oG _.ooo _.owo 9o— 98 NAN _.ooq 98w Ab; 588030: Zona— Ume 90m 93 98 _.omo 9A3 whom .98 chm 93 9a.: 9AAw #qu wz: -90A 98 9Aa 93A 930 _.o: -93 90A :3 903 9a: HbMA >mo 93 9: 9mm #33 9m: _.mww o.oo 9S 9AM _.ooo 9wAm _.AS ZmC Cmo 98 9o. Ab? _.Sa _.oco Ab”: 93 o.oo MUN _.ooq 993 _.So Umzwx ch C8 93 9S 9”: _.ooq o.oww _.owA -92 9S 9; 98a 99: _.oo_ _._ w A .8. «so-8:09 .3 h A .9. "So-8:09 223 Females. The Additive Model with perceptions of the average MSU student’s alcohol consumption was not significant as an overall model (38(4) = 6.00, p = .200, see Table 63, p. 223). The Interaction Model also was not significant as an overall model (36(5) = 9.293, p = .093). Dijferenfiation &Perception of Best Friend at MS U ’s Alcohol Use Males. The Additive Model with perceptions of their best friend at MSU’s alcohol consumption was significant (38(4) = 25.61, p < .001, RIC“ & gm“ = .13, Rznmu‘m = .22) and had a successful prediction rate of 82.7%, which was 5% less than having no model. Students that had one unit higher in perceptions of best friend use were 1.028 (95% CI = 1.013, 1.043) times more likely to be a Binge Drinker versus a Nonbinging Drinker (see Table 64, p. 225). Students that had a higher level of perception of best friend use equivalent to one standard deviation (72.43) were 7.067 times more likely to be a Binge Drinker. The Interaction Model also was significant (78(5) = 25.68, p < .001, RIC... & s...“ = .13, Rznmmm = .22) and had a successful prediction rate of 82.7%, but did not show a block improvement over the Additive Model (380) = .16, p = .693). The only significant individual predictor in the model was perception of best friend use (OR = 1.028, 95% CI = 1.013, 1.043, see Table 64, p. 225). 224 Haw—a i rommmmo Womnnmmmo: wagon—gm 958$ 92 m8 gamma 588 232508 I men—.8393 cm wow" «.1053 Can _l Zaom _ moBEom _ a _ mm _ éaa fl ox _ 3.x. 9 _ a _ mmF éoa _ ox _ 3.x. 9 W83 30%. Ume -98 9g 93 9on 93m _.mmo .93 93 93 9mg 93m _.wma 95 -98 93 9B obi 92$ roam -93 93 N8 903 93o _.ooo >mo 98 93 98 _._HX 93m _.mao 93 95 9; _.omm 93m _.wao >355 Zona— Umzw 93 93 9S _.owq 980 N. _ mo -9 _ .\ 98 9“: 9%3 93o _.MNA 95 -93 o.oo 9% 903 9me _.o.: -98 92 _.No 903 93w _.8.» >mo o.oo 9; 98 room 9qu _.u: 93 9E o.oo #ko 9.3m _.uau wow” «125$ C3 98 93 3.3.... _.owm _.Sw roam 93 93 ~93: rouo _.omu _.omm 58330: zona— Umzw 9mm 93 9 D m _.Nmm 93o Pug -93 93 93 9.25 93% _.3a 95 -93 o.oo 93 903 9mg _.Sm -93 93 _.No 99% 93m _bAo >mo o.oo 9 _ a 9g _.oom 9.5a rm: 9% 9 _ A 98 $vo 9.3m #89 wow» 3.5:: :3 98 93 5.21... _.oum _.Su _.ofi. 93 92 3.31. _.OAo _.owu _.omu Um?” x womamnosaw C8 93 9...: 93 roam 928 _.9: -93 92 9.3 90$ 93» _.oz a. to A .3. 25-8:09 .1 No A .3. EGAN—=09 225 Females. The Additive Model with perceptions of their best friend at MSU’s alcohol consumption was significant (12(4) = 45.28, p < .001, R2“, & Sm" = .19, Rzuwkflh = .27) and had a successful prediction rate of 75.6%, an increase 5.6% over the Basic Model. Students that had one tulit higher in perceptions of best friend use were 1.039 (95% CI = 1.023, 1.055) times more likely to be a Binge Drinker versus a Nonbinging Drinker (see Table 64, p. 225). Students that had a higher level of perception of best friend use equivalent to one standard deviation (41.48) were 4.84 times more likely to be a Binge Drinker versus a Nonbinging Drinker. The Interaction Model also was significant (752(5) = 46.02, p < .001, 14220,,( a 5.," = .19, RZNW = .27) and had a successful prediction rate of 76.1%, but did not show a block improvement over the Additive Model (12(1) = .74, p = .39). The only significant individual predictor in the model was perception of best friend use, which was nearly identical in value as the Additive Model (OR = 1.040, 95% CI = 1.023, 1.057, see Table 64, p. 225). Dijfirentiation & C 0A Status Males. The Additive Model with COA status failed to reach a level of significance (76(4) = 1.23, p = .873, see Table 65, p. 227). The Interaction Model also failed to achieve a significance as an overall model (x26) = 1.359, p = .929). Females. The Additive Model with COA status failed to reach a level of significance (38(4) = 4.200, p = .380, see Table 65, p. 227). The Interaction Model also failed to achieve a significance as an overall model 086) = 5.033, p = .412). 226 .320 am rommmmo Womaommmos wRamozam £595 93 8d wmamoa mSEm _ 3&8 _ moaaom _ a _ mmP drama 4 ow _ 8.x. 9 _ a # mm m 55 _ ow _ 8.x. 9 momma 30%: 62% -93 93 98 9on 93m _.mmo .93 93 9% 98M 93m _.wmm $5 -98 93 9B 903 9me _.omm -93 93 New 9er 9on _.ooo >mo 9 _ u 9 _ m o.oo :3 93m _.mmo 9cm 9 _ u 9 _ a _.omm 93m :3 >335 gono— Uwvw -93 93 93 93A 93a _.05 -98 93 9% 9m 2 m 9.28 _.wqo E’s: -98 93 9 _ a 928 9m? #23 -93 9E 93 93A 9me _.oom >mo 9B 93 9% _.SA 9me 32 9am 9B 9B _.oao 93m 53 00> -93 9mm 93 93c 930 930 -93 98 93 93; 93a 2.9.8 58398: Kong GB.” -9 _ u of 9 _o 9me 9qu 2.03 -93 98 :m 9S” 9.5m _.Noo Ex: -98 93 9 _ m 926 953 2:3 -93 93 98 98h 9m? _.ooa >mo 93 93 o.oo :um 9Su 32 93 9G 98 _.oou 930 _.wS 00> -93 9% 93 98w 93m N. H cm -93 9a: _.3 93A 9MB :3; Ume x 00> 9X 93 9S 2.3m 9»: PM: 93 93 93 2.9.3 93m 98a _._.u A .3. 30-3559 3h A .S. 95-8:09 227 Dzfl'erentiation & Expectations of Alcohol Serving as a Tension Reducer Males. The Additive Model with tension reduction expectations was significant as an overall model 08(4) = 20.74, p < .001, RICO, & s..." = .11, RIM“... = .18). The model had a successful prediction rate of 83.2%, which was identical to having no model. Expectation of alcohol serving as a tension reducer was the only significant individual variable. Students that had a one unit higher level of expectation were 1.133 (95% CI = 1.068, 1.202) times more likely to be a Binge Drinker versus a Nonbinging Drinker (see Table 66, p. 229). Students that had a greater perception equal to one standard deviation (8.73) were 2.98 times more likely to be Binge Drinkers. The Interaction Model also was significant overall (38(5) = 22.13, p < .001, RICO, & 5m“ = .11, Rsz = .19) and had a successful prediction rate of 83.2%; however, it did not show a block improvement over the Additive Model (12(1) = 1.39, p = .24). Expectations of tension reduction remained the only significant individual predictor (OR = 1.142, 95% CI = 1.073, 1.216) of Binge Drinking (see Table 66, p. 229). Females. The Additive Model with tension reduction expectations was significant as an overall model (38(4) = 43.71, p < .001, RZCO,‘ & 5nd1= .19, Rznwak, = .26). The model had a successful prediction rate of 74.2%, an increase of 5.7% over not having a model. Expectations of alcohol serving as a tension reducer changed the odds of being a Binge Drinker versus a Nonbinging Drinker by 1.147 (95% CI = 1.094, 1.202) for a one unit difference in expectations (see Table 66, p. 229). By changing the difference in expectations to a unit equivalent to one standard deviation for expectations (8.81) the likelihood of being a Binge Drinker is 3.34 times greater. 228 Hugo mm roman.” Wannommmoa 385:8 mzanam EN: Ba 9803 mo 9 _ u 9 H a 98 :85 98m _.mg 98 9 m u 9 _ m _.omm 98m _.uAc 25:78 30%; Um?” 98 9mm _.Nq _.muo 98“ 8. 3w 98 9.3 98 _.NAM 98— MES 95 92 98 98 _.oou 988 :8 -98 98 :N 98m. 93m _.OAN >mo 9B 9; T: _.8m 9m.: _.qmw 93 9: 93 :8 980 _.Ama H255: Waggon 9; 98 3.8.1. :8 room _.Nom 9: 98 8.8.... :3 _.8» _.Nou 388030: Zona— Umzw 9.: 93 Na; Nona 9?: +me 98 9mm 98 _.3.» 93¢ No8 BE 93 98 98 _.80 908 _._NA -98 98 9mm 98» 93A fog >mo 98 9; _.No _.NNN 9mg _.dm 9: 9E 98 :8 9mm_ _.gm H258: wagons 98 98 3.8%.... :3 _.oqu _.Ba 93 98 89mm: :3 room Ewen Umzw X Adamo: Woacomoa 98 98 _.8 _.oao 93A :8 -98 90; N: 995 9mg. _.owo _._ m A .8, 9293—8. .1. .u A .3. go-§_oa. 229 The Interaction Model also was significant overall (38(5) = 45.96, p < .001, RICOx & 5...,“ = .19, Rznmlkm = .27) and had a successful prediction rate of 71.8%; however, it did not show a block improvement over the Additive Model 080) = 2.25, p = .134). Expectations of tension reduction remained the only significant individual predictor (OR = 1.146, 95% CI = 1.092, 1.202, see Table 66, p. 229). Diflerentiation & Expectations of Alcohol Serving as a Social Lubricant Males. The Additive Model with social lubrication expectations was significant as an overall model (38(4) = 19.43, p = .001, R20”, & Sod. = .10, Rzmgcumk. = .17). The model had a successful prediction rate of 83.8%, an increase of .6% over the not having a model. The only significant individual predictor was expectations of alcohol serving as a social lubricant. Students that had a one unit higher level of expectation were 1.145 (95% CI = 1.071, 1.224) times more likely to be a Binge Drinker versus a Nonbinging Drinker (see Table 67, p. 233). Students that had a greater perception equal to one standard deviation (7.89) were 2.93 times more likely to be Binge Drinkers. The Interaction Model also was significant overall (38(5) = 19.58, p = .001, R20... & 5...," = .10, Rznmu‘m = .17) and had a successful prediction rate of 82.7%; however, it did not show a block improvement over the Additive Model (380) = .15, p = .697). Expectations for social lubrication remained the only significant individual predictor (OR = 1.147, 95% CI = 1.072, 1.227) in predicting Bingers versus Nonbinging Drinkers. Females. The Additive Model with social lubrication expectations was significant as an overall model (38(4) = 17.33, p = .002, R20, .1 5...... = .08, mm... = .11). The model had a successful prediction rate of 70%, an increase of 1.5% over the not having a model. Expectations of alcohol serving as a social lubricant changed the odds of being a 230 drinker by 1.090 (95% CI = 1.040, 1.142) for every one unit change in perceptions (see Table 67, p. 233). Students with a difference in expectations equal to one standard deviation (7.74) were 1.95 times more likely to be a Binge Drinker versus a Nonbinging Drinker. The Interaction Model also was significant overall (78(5) = 18.07, p = .003, RZCO,‘ & 5...," = .08, fireman“,e = .11) and had a successful prediction rate of 71 .8%; however, it did not show a block improvement over the Additive Model (36(1) = .74, p = .3 s9). Expectations for social lubrication remained the only significant individual predictor (OR = 1.089, 95% CI = 1.039, 1.141) in predicting Binge Drinkers versus Nonbinging Drinkers (see Table 67, p. 233). The Comprehensive Model Males. The Comprehensive Model was significant according to the model chi- square (21203) = 41.1 1, p < 001.11%,” 3.....= .20, Rimm. = .34). The model illustrated an 85.4% success rate in predicting the correct drinking classifications, a 2.2% increase over having no model. Perceptions of best fiiend use (OR = 1.019) was the only individual variables to significantly improve the odds of being classified as a Binge Drinker versus a Nonbinging Drinker (see Table 68, p. 234). Females. The Comprehensive Model was significant according to the model chi- square (3603) = 85.81, p < .001, 112.2,... 5,... = .33, RIM...” = .47). The model illustrated a 70% success rate in predicting the correct drinking classifications, the identical value to the Basic Model. Perceptions of best fiiend use (OR = 1.037), expectations of tension reduction (OR = l. 166), and the differentiation X expectations of 231 tension reduction (OR = .862) were the only individual variables to significantly improve the odds of being classified as a Binge Drinker versus a Nonbinging Drinker. Occasional Binge Drinkers versus Nonbinging Drinkers Diflerentiation and binge drinking Males. The Basic Model was not significant according to the model chi-square (38(3) = .50 p = .919, see Table 69, p. 236). Females. The Basic Model did not reach a level of significance (38(3) = 1.40, p = .705, see Table 69, p. 236). F our- factors of the DSI-R with binge drinking Males. As was the case in the Basic Model, the overall four-factor model failed to reach a level of significance (38(6) = 2.77, p = .838, see Table 70, p. 236). Females. The overall four-factor model failed to reach a level of significance (36(6) = 4.58, p = .599, see Table 70, p. 236). Dijferentiation & perception of average MS U student ’s alcohol use Males. The Additive Model failed to reach a level of significance (36(4) = 1.547, p = .818, see Table 71, p. 237). Similarly, the Interaction Model also was not significant as an overall model (38(5) = 2.980, p = .703). Females. The Additive Model with perceptions of the average MSU student’s alcohol consumption was not significant as an overall model (36(4) = 3.36, p = .500, see Table 71, p. 237). The Interaction Model also was not significant as an overall model (38(5) = 4.273, p = .511, see Table 71, p. 237). 232 Hmzm 8 homage Womanmmmoa 32:35 9338 93 m3 Esme; <22; Zoacmsmaa l mxvooamaoaw om moor: 9.51830: fl Kan—om _ mans—mm _ o _ mm _ éoa _ ow _ been. 8 a fi mm _ sin _ Q: 3:. Q momma Zone Um?” -98 98 98 908 988 _.mwo -98 98 9am 9m8 9&8 _.uma 9,3 -98 98 9B 903 9me #88 -98 98 8.8 908 980 _.oo0 >8 9 _ u 9 3 98 :8 988 _.mao 98 9 8 w 9 H m _.08 988 830 >338 Zona— Uwzw 98 98 _.Nm _.m-fi 98; 93 q 9 _ w 980 98— :fi 98_ _ .000 BE 98 98 98 _.S m 908 :80 -98 98 8.3 902 980 H .8 _ .98 98 9; _.8 _.88 930 _.qwq 93 9B 98 :9 988 _.80 moan: r5185: 9 3 98 8.8.1. :8 #0.: _.8A 900 98 8.31. _.o0o _.80 _.EN 58388: Zona— Umzw 98 98 _.8 _.23 9o00 98m 98 980 98 _.ofi 98.; ~80; Eé 98 98 98 _.S m 908 :80 -98 98 _.~_ 908 9%: :08 >8 98 9 _ x _.mm _.88 980 _.08 9 _ o 9 _ u 9%. C _o 98¢ :88 mega rccnomnos 9 I 98 8.0%.... :3 _.08 _.NN-\ 900 98 8.3.3.. _.80 _.80 :3 Ume x moan—reggae: 98 98 98 _.8_ 90M— :8 -98 98 98 908 9w0¢ _.og. _.. h A .8. "fie-82mm. _._... m A .or ”So-3:3. 233 Hugo 8 rommmao wamammmo: 32:38 manna—8 :5. Ed Emma? <22; 205E803 I 0036832588 Zona— Zaam moBEom 8 mm $55 0w 8.8 Q . 8 mm ’58 OW 088 Q Um?” 98 98 _.8 N. _ 8 980 m. _ 8 -9 _ u 98 98 988 988 N. ~ 08 9’: 9o— 98 9o. _.o8 9m0~ :8 -98 98 _.N0 908 988 _.8N >ma 9 _ a 9B 98 _.NS 0808 _.m fl 8 9 _ 0 9 _ q _.8 _.N8 988 _.80 78C 00800325 92 92 _.N0 _.o: 908 _.ouo o.oo 93 9.8 _.08 908 :08 Um?” x 286 0380303 92 98 98 :08 98c :08 -98 92 who 908 908 _.08 mam“ manna 98 93 8.8... 8:0 :08 :08 98 92 8.8.... _.08 _.8o _.88 Um?” x mom" wing 98 92 98 _.oou 980 #88 -98 9S _.8 908 9080 _.08 00> -_.8 9-2 who 98w 988 ~08 -98 98: N8 988 980 :3 Um?” X 00> 98 _.No 980 _.08 988 N988 -98 98 98 908 98_ 8.88 H250: ”@9830: 98 98 _.8 #88 908 :8 98 98 8.8.3.. :8 _.08 .88— szw x 838: ”@9830: 98 98 9.5 _.80 908 _.80 -98 98 8.8... 988 9.88 908 moor: r5188: 98 98 r00 #88 98c :00 -92 98 98 908 930 78¢ Um?” x moaa rccaomao: -92 98 9S 900a 988 _.80 98 98 98 :08 908 _.N8 *5 A .8. Eco-8:3. _._... h A .3. "So-8:3. 234 Dijferentiation & Perception of Best Friend at MSU ’3 Alcohol Use Males. The Additive Model failed to reach a level of significance (38(4) = 6.130, p = .190, see Table 72, p. 238). The Interaction Model also failed to reach a level of significance (38(5) = 6.219, p = .286, see Table 72, p. 233). Females. The Additive Model with perceptions of their best friend at MSU’s alcohol consumption was significant (78(4) = 15.09, p < .001, RIC“ & 5,," = .11, RZNmumc = .15) and had a successful prediction rate of 66.9%, an increase 16.2% over the Basic Model. Students that had one unit higher in perceptions of best fi'iend use were 1.026 (95% CI = 1.009, 1.042) times more likely to be an Occasional Binge Drinker versus a Nonbinging Drinker (see Table 72, p. 238). Students that had a higher level of perception of best fi'iend use equivalent to one standard deviation (41.48) were 2.82 times more likely to be an Occasional Binge Drinker versus a Nonbinging Drinker. The Interaction Model also was significant (78(5) = 16.62, p = .005, RIC... & 5,... = .11, RZNWe = .15) and had a successful prediction rate of 66.9%, but did not show a block improvement over the Additive Model (12(1) = .12, p = .724). The only significant individual predictor in the model remained perception of best friend use, which was nearly identical in value as the Additive Model (OR = 1.025, 95% CI = 1.009, 1.042, see Table 72, p. 238). Dlfierentiation & COA Status Males. The Additive Model failed to reach a level of significance (38(4) = .59, p = .96, see Table 73, p. 239). The Interaction Model also failed to reach a level of significance (76(5) = .66, p = .985, see Table 73, p. 239). 235 Hugo ac rommmzo Womammmos 39:35 wficmmim :5" can 088ng gamma <22; 2039508 I 92083330: Zaom «nae—om 6 mm find—E OW ome\o Q 6 mm «Sea 0” 8X 9 Ume chm obo of» _.wNA 0.2: NSN .95 cm; 9.3 onao ob: _.wmo wZH o.oo o.oM o.oo 99% 9mg 2 I -98 o.oc ouw o.owo 93w fog >mo .98 93 0.3 903 9ko _.woo o.om 93 PM; _.owm PmNm _.ANa ate A .8. "$5-8:me in A .3. 23-x: on. How—o .5 bommmao anqommmoa 383:5 manners. :5» m8 08853. wmsmoa 585 Zoacmnmoa l mos. 1883 3E8 moan—flow 6 mm (<39 OW 3.x. 9 9 mm fins—E OW 3.x. Q magmas». wowomSQ -93 of 9: 9w: chow #03 Pom obo PS #on ob: _.oom J: menace chm coo TS #qu 9qu “$3 -93 Pm» New PM: oboe _.oua wagon». 0:8? PNN 93 93 #th ob: Nuwm ob» ohm 9mm rwaa @qu No: 558: in. 0963 -93 chi o.oo 9on obs PAS .98 93 Pom 903 ohm“ _.oa w?= -obw o.oo o.oo 903 Pm: room .93 o.oo _.3 90% Puma— #80 >ma -obA obo o.om o.oum Pat. {bu o. fl o o. 3 ohm :8 PEG _.Aua a .u A bu. 35-3.8. IbA .2. 35-3159 236 flmEo .2 rommmao Womammmoa 32:35 £525 :8: Ed 0888:»— wwsmoaw <28; Zoaumsmoa 1 wonoouaoam om ch manna Cm» :88 _ won—£8 8 :3 €65 _ ox _ 89.9 _ 8 _mm _ £2: ox _ 889 wwmmo gong em; 98 98 9.3 $8 99: $8 -98 9: 98 98o 9a: 58 we: 98 98 98 988 88 _._: -98 98 98 98o 88 :5 >8 -98 98 98 99: 9an C8 98 9: 98. S8 988 :8 >aa58 -98 9B 98 98a 98» $8 98 9: 98 23 9x; :8 78¢ can 92 92 98 S8 998 :8 92 9S _.8 S8 9er S; 38330: 325 am; 93 98 _.8 38 993 988 -9: 9: 98 98a 988 58 we: 98 98 9S 98o 98¢ 28 -98 98 98 9o: $8 98““ >8 -98 98 98 98“ 986 C8 98 9: 98 _.8_ 986 2.: am: can 92 92 9t 28 923 _.8a 92 92 _.2 S8 98 28 3:; 78: can 98 98 _.8 :5 923 IE -92 92 98 98 9c: :8 _._fi A .3. 35.823. .1. .8 A .3. "So-8:8. 237 Hugo .5 rommmzo ”3385: 38838 9338 98 m8 088338 gamma 885.. 20:28on I $80388 om mom” man—aw can _ 3&8 _ mange—am _ 8 3mm _ sea _ ox _ 889 a 8 _ mm _ éoa _ ow 9 8.89 m8? Zona— UmH-W 98 93 9.6 _.wNA 9m3 905 -98 93 98 9.8a 9.2; 3.80 35 o.oo 98 o.oo 908 9mg 3 Z -98 98 98 93c 98m 90% >8 -98 98 98 983 988 88 98 9 3 A 9: _.omm 98m 3.3m .2535 Zeal UmZN 93 9A— 98 _.Am-N o.oon 8.8.: -93 98 o.oo 98A 988 _.wow 2": -93 o.oo 93 98a 9me room -98 98 9% 93¢ 98o _.otw >8 -93 93 93 93c 93a _.38 98 9G 93 reg 93w {Ga wow” magma Gun 98 93 PK... 33 m _.o3 98¢ 98 93 9: a... _.ono _.ooo 995 588030: Zona— UmH-W 9% 9.3 o.oo _.ucx 928 No.3 -98 980 98 9%: 9w _ m _.Aum E": -93 98 98 998 98a :8 -98 98 9?. 93¢ 9ch 903 >mo -9: 9B 93 980 93a _.w: 98 9 _ m 98 #28 9x _ m _.aw wow» magma 9.6 98 93 Ana... 93¢ _.o3 _.8_ 98 93 951. 9on race _.OAN 03-x x wow» magma Cmo o.oo 93 o.oo :5; 993 H.8N -93 93 9S 93m 90% _.cww sh A .8. 36.828. _._... N» A .3. 35.8on. 238 Ania d rommmao wamaammmoa 3.23953 3:538 35» man 008925. wmsmaam 888 20:33:33 I 00> 388m _ 3&3 _ maBEom _ 3 _mm _€m_a90x_ 3:9 _ @ _mm_€%_ 9: 3:9 wgmo gono— UMZN 9mm 93 93 _.uNA 938 PEN -93 9B 98 983 93A #33 wzm 933 98 93 983 933 _._: -98 93 933 933 933 9.33 >mo -98 93 98 903 933 “.303 98 9: 93; 9.38 933 #33 >335 Kean. Um?” 93 93 933 _.83 938 No3 -93 93— 93 938 933 9.30 w?= o.oo 933 o.oo _.333 933 :5 -98 93 93 9033 9303 .33 >mo -98 93 933 903 933 _.303 93 9: 98 _.33 93: _.AE 00> -93 933 93 933 983 N33 -92 93 _.33 938 983 _.33 583030: gono— UmEN 9m» 93 93 3.33 9H3 3.33 -95 98 933 935 933 3.38 W73 93 933 o.oo 903 933 _._: -98 93A 933 9030 9303 _.33 >mo -98 93 93 983 933 _.3~ 93 9: 98 _.3N 93: _.AE 00> -98 933 93 93: 9N3 N30 -93 93 3.3 933 9qu _.Aoo UMH-WX 00> 9mm _33 93 _.38 93.3 3.3: -98 98 o.oo 983 9:6 A33 *5 A .8. "so-8:8. 2.. w A .or «fie-8:09 239 Females. The Additive Model with COA status failed to reach a level of significance (12(4) = 2.792, p = .593, see Table 73, p. 239). The Interaction Model also failed to achieve a significance as an overall model (38(5) = 2.795, p = .732). Difierentiation & Expectations of Alcohol Serving as a Tension Reducer Males. The Additive Model failed to reach a level of significance (x2(4) = 8.94, p = .063, see Table 74, p. 242). The Interaction Model was significant as an overall model (x2(5)=11-19,p = .048, slams“: .12, Rimm, = .16) and had a successful prediction rate of 75.8%. Although differentiation approached significance (p = .052), expectations of tension reduction remained the only significant individual predictor (OR = 1.108, 95% CI = 1.030, 1.191) of Occasional Binge Drinkers versus Nonbinging Drinkers (see Table 74, p. 242). Females. The Additive Model with tension reduction expectations was significant as an overall model (38(4) = 21.62, p < .001, RZCOuSndl = .14, R214“: = .19). The model had a successful prediction rate of 66.2%, an increase of 14.8% over not having a model. Expectations of alcohol serving as a tension reducer changed the odds of being an Occasional Binge Drinker versus a Nonbinging Drinker by 1.117 (95% CI = 1.060, 1.177) for a one unit difl‘erence in expectations (see Table 74, p. 242). By changing the difference in expectations to a unit equivalent to one standard deviation for expectations (8.81) there is a 4.13 times greater likelihood of being anOccasional Binge Drinker. The Interaction Model also was significant overall (38(5) = 24.15, p < .001, RIC“ & 5...," = .19, Rznmn‘m = .27) and had a successful prediction rate of 64. 1%; however, it did not show a block improvement over the Additive Model (22(1) = 2.52, p = .112). 240 Expectations of tension reduction remained the only significant individual predictor (OR = 1.114, 95% CI = 1.056, 1.176, see Table 74, p. 242). Diflcrentiation & Expectations of A lcohol Serving as a Social Lubricant Males. The Additive Model with social lubrication expectations was significant as an overall model (78(4) = 10.12, p = .039, ale,” 3...“ = .11, Emma... = .15). The model had a successfill prediction rate of 70.3%, an increase of 4.4% over the not having a model. The only significant individual predictor was expectations of alcohol serving as a social lubricant. Students that had a one unit higher level of expectation were 1.118 (95% CI = 1.036, 1.206) times more likely to be an Occasional Binge Drinker versus a Nonbinging Drinker (see Table 75, p. 243). Students that had a greater perception equal to one standard deviation (7.89) were 2.40 times more likely to be Occasional Binge Drinkers. The Interaction Model failed to reach a level of significance (13(5) = 10.21, p = .070) and had a successful prediction rate of 71.4%. Expectations for social lubrication remained the only significant individual predictor for the sample (OR = 1.118, 95% CI = 1.036, 1.206) in predicting Occasional Binge Drinkers versus Nonbinging Drinkers (see Table 75, p. 243). Females. The Additive Model failed to reach a level of significance as an overall model (38(4) = 6.094, p = .192, see Table 75, p. 243). The Interaction Model also failed to reach a level of significance (38(5) = 7.056, p = .216). The Comprehensive Model Males. The Comprehensive Model failed to reach a level of significance (x203) = 19.269, p = .115, see Table 76, p. 245). 241 Hugo .3 rommmno ”anaemic: 32:35 magmas. 5m» man Oonmmmosfi gamma <22; Zoacmsmonm l mxuoofimosm om. Adamo: Won—come: _ 2&8 _ moBEam _ a _ mm _ sea _ ow _ 8&9 _ a _ a: 55 _ ow _ 8&9 wean Zena DE.” 98 9.8 93 8mm 92: 905 -98 9“: 98 98m 9A: 88 El: o.oo 98 o.oo 93m 98.» _._ E -98 98 9mm 99.5 998 _.oao >ma -98 93 98 993 988 83 98 9: 98A _.omm 93m _.ama >355 332 02% 98 98 98 Emma 9.3m 988 -98 98 92 903 9m8 _.mmo ES— 98 98 98 _.8A 903 :8 -98 98 9 3 998 92: _.cum >mo -93 9B 98 93m 92m _.Aoo 9; 9G _.qu :3 9mg rm: Anammo: Waggon 93 98 98...... :8 _.ouq :8 o; _ 98 3.81;. _._ 3 _.oao :3 58398: Zona— Umzw _.oo 9% 8.3 NQNN 90m: 93¢ -9»; 9mm 98 9:3 983 _.aoo ave: 98 o.oo 98 _.80 903 :8 -93 98 93 90mm 98w _.owm .98 -95 92 98A 923 9m8 _.wt 9; 9G _.3 _.N8 9mg _.omn Hosea: ”3:98: 98 98 98...... :8 E80 :3 9: 98 3.3.... C E memo :uo Um?” x fiasco: Waggon 98 98 PM» font: 93A _.NNN -93 98 who 98o 9mg _.Sm _._ me A .8. 26-889 _._... E A .o 7 25-889 242 Haze 38 hommmao woman-$3: 33838 93038 33 80 08382: 959.5 58% 22.3803 1 mxvooSmoam em wooE reggae: 2E8 a 12:38 a a :m _ éaa _ owfl axe _ e _mm _ fie: 9: case 888 Zona— Ume 98 93 9A0 _.uNA 92: N05 -98 9”: 98 938 9A: _.wmo W72 o.oo 98 o.oo 900m 9m3 _._ _ _ -98 98 9% 93¢ 9m0m _.80 >8 -98 9.8 98 903 998 _.88 98 9 E 980 _.08 988 ~08 >833 Zona— UmH-W 900 98 Pt _ .000 98; A308 -900 93 93 90 _ 0 93m _.33 m2: 98 98 98A #8.. 908 :3 -98 98 9N0 93m 908 .08 >mo -92 9B 98 903 988 ~03 98 9: 9M: :80 980 _.800 mega Enigma: 9: 98 98...... Z 5 _.8o _.Nca 98 98 #8. _.8m _.ooa _._ 2 38898: Zone Gm?” 93 93 N3 MLE 930.. 8.38 -98 98 9.: 9303 980a _.aow ES— 98 98 98 _.08 908 :8 -98 90A 9 _ 3 908 903 _.08 >8 -98 9B 93 90M: 980 _.Awo 9: 9E 9m0 :8 98A _.m_0 meow». 551830: 9: 98 9mm: : a 0.8m _.N3 98 98 Fun... _.88 _.oou _._ _o Umrw x wage—haulage: 98 98 98 _.30 908 :8 -98 98 98 98A 980 roam an A .8. 3.5-8:8. new A .8. vac-8:8. 243 Females. The Comprehensive Model was significant according to the model chi- square (x203) = 45.05, p < .001, R2c°x & 5...,- = .27, RIM-m. = .36) and showed a significant improvement over the Basic Model 0800) = 43.65, p < .001). The model illustrated a 74.6% success rate in predicting the correct drinking classifications, an improvement of 23.9% over the Basic Model. Perceptions of best friend use (OR = 1.030), expectations of tension reduction (OR = 1.147), and the difl'erentiation X expectations of tension reduction (OR = .861) were the only individual variables to significantly improve the odds of being classified as an Occasional Binge Drinker versus a Nonbinging Drinker (see Table 76, p. 245). Frequent Binge Drinkers versus Nonbinging Drinkers Dlfierentiation and binge drinking Males. The Basic Model was not significant according to the model chi-square (78(3) = 2.48, p = .479, see Table 77, p. 246). Females. The Basic Model was not significant according to the model chi-square (x20) = 5.81, p = .121, see Table 77, p. 246). F our- factors of the DSI-R with binge drinking Males. As was the case in the Basic Model, the overall four-factor model failed to reach a level of significance (12(6) = 3.78, p = .706, see Table 78, p. 246). Females. The overall four-factor model failed to reach a level of significance (x2(6) = 7.39.12 = .286). 244 H35 .3 homage Womamao: wnommoaam mamas? Em» 30 0883:»— wEMo—a <99; Zoacmnmanm I OoBEoroammé Zona— Zaom Edam—om u mm €95 OW 8.x. 9 w mm 955 CW 3.x. Q Ume -93 9% “.8 93a 9 a AN flhoa -93 9mm 93 923 9mg .2 u wz: 9 # a 98 NS :3 93m _.Aoa -93 93 93 993 9%: rod >mo 98 93 93 _.omq 9q~m _.aww 9B 9: r3 _.an 93w _.‘Do Em: 38828.4. o.oo 93 92 980 923 _.SN 93 93 93 _.com 93; re; 02% x ch waaobaosm -98 92 Wow 993 98c _.oou -93 93 98 98. 903 _.So wow» «123 98 9o— 5?“ _.owa 5o: _.oww 98 9S 9N5: _.owo I: o fog ‘39 Ume x mam" $55 -93 93 93 93M 98m re; -93 98 93 93m 90% _.So 00> -Za 99). who 9w; 993 _.oqa -93 9mm _.ao 9%: 93a _.ANN 02%, X 00> _.ww _._m _.uw Put 9mg $.25 -93 93 93 9%: 9:6 .33 Adamo: ”098:0: 90¢ 93 93 rowq 98a :3 9E 93 3.8... :3 _.oau _.Nuw § 02% x Anzac: ”3.830: 9: 9: o.oo _._ G 9m£ 7qu -9; 93 a; _._ 9M3— 9.Rm 98m mega r5183: -93 93 98 903 93w _.omm -98 9E 9: 93m o.ooo fog Ume x moomm— r5183: -93 93 9B 9?: 93¢ _.Now 98 98 9mm _.oma 9o: rmmm _._ u A .8. 25-8:8. _._... E A .2. 25-823. 245 ngo .3 rommmao Womammmo: 32:33 mange :5" m3 33:9: wmamaa 835m Zoacmsmoa I 0520:3303 3&8 moaaam w mm 9&5 OW 3.x. 9 v mm «SHE OW 8.x. 9 Umzw -93 9mm 93 9‘2 9?: _.MNA -95 9mm 93 9me 93A _.omw ES“ -93 o.oo 9mm 93m 93w rouo -93 93 PB... 9on 93m 90: >mo 9- 9: _.3 _.Ng 9mg ruww 98. 9G 93 _.Sm 9am _.wmo _._ A bu. 25-8:3. 2;» A .3. 2:13—09 Haw—a uw homage Womnommmos vammomsm mange 9E 98 man—52 9508 838 Zea—gmamoa I «9: 3888 3&8 moans—om w mm €95 OW 8.x. 9 v mm £35 ow 3.x. 9 mBomoaa WomomiQ 9mm 9mm 9mm rwum 93m ~03 9G 98 9N” :fi 93m _.mfi 2H: vomaos .98 93 9mm 9mm. 93» _.mqa 93 9mm o.oo re: 9903 _.EN magmas»— OEom. -93 9u~ 93 9H3 93a Emma 98 98 98 room o.ouo _.ud mama: £9 0:83 .95 9.8 9? 93w 93o rcmm -93 9: _.3 9?: 93a ”bum BE -93 98 9% 93m 9mm.» 2:: -9 _ w 93 a. _ m. 93m 93; 93m >mo 98 9: E8 _.Nwm 923 _.qmw 98 9G 98 _.omo 9&0 3,: _._ h A .omv 25-8%9 IE A .3. 25.4359 246 Dijflrentiation & perception of average MS U student ’3 alcohol use Males. The Additive Model was significant as an overall model (x2(4) = 10.96, p = .027, R20”, & 3...,“ = .08, [€sz = .13). The model had a successful prediction rate of 76%, an improvement of .8% over having no model. Perceptions of MSU student use was the only significant individual predictor. Students that had a one unit higher level of perception were 1.022 (95% CI = 1.004, 1.040) times more likely to be Frequent Binge Drinkers than Nonbinging Drinkers (see Table 79, p. 248). Students that had a greater perception equal to one standard deviation (37.22) were 2.27 times more likely to be Frequent Binge Drinkers. The Interaction Model was significant as an overall model (12(5) = 11.20, p = .048, R2“x & 5...," = .09, Rznwm = .13), had a successful prediction rate of 74.4%; however, it did not show a block improvement over the Additive Model (380) = .24, p = .624). Perceptions of MSU student alcohol use remained the only significant individual variable in predicting Frequent Binge Drinkers versus Nonbinging Drinkers (OR = 1.023, 95% CI = 1.005, 1.043, see Table 79, p. 248). Females. The Additive Model with perceptions of the average MSU student’s alcohol consumption was not significant as an overall model (12(4) = 7.850, p = .097, see Table 79, p. 248). The Interaction Model was significant as an overall model (13(5) = 14.01, p = .016, RICO, & 5m“ = .10, Rzumn‘m = .13) and had a significant block improvement over the Additive Model (x2(1)= 6.16, p = .013). The model had a successful prediction rate of 63%, which was a 4.3% improvement over the Basic Model and 11.6% over having no model. The differentiation X perception of MSU student use was the only significant 247 #63 ‘3 rommmao Womammmoa vaoamflim $5.38 :5” 2.0 33:02 wmnmoa mo 9- 9 H u _.i _.Nao 9wo~ rumw 9S 9 _ m 93 _.S m 9.3m _.umo 25:76 Zona— Umzw -98 9.5 9.3 930 93a _.Ew 9% 9w» 93 93 x 93m _.uwm mg“ -98 93 :3 90: 9mg _.ouc -9 _ N 98 93 9a: 9.30 :5“. >mo 93 9; NS _.Noo 98m _.mum 98 9G 9? 2H: 93m _.wmu ch C8 98 9S o.oo. _.oum _.ooa fog 93 9S _.8 _.ooq 903 _.o: 38898: :32 Um?” -9 _ m 93 9: 93; 9a.: No: -9NA 9w» 9km 93o 9.8m #qu 95 -98 93 :3 93 m 9mg _.oao -9 B 93 Pun 933 9.3a room >ma 9g 9; NE “New 903 _.wwq 98 93 93 room 93o TEN ch C3 98 9E 93... fonu _.oom #96 93 93 Now _.oom 903 _.So Ume X ch Gun 93 98 9~A rcow 903 _.owo -98 93 9%... 93m 98m 908 _._Lu A .3. 25-859 _._... w A .S. go-$=om. 248 individual predictor (see Table 79, p. 248, and Figure 6, p. 73, for interaction effect). Students with a one unit higher interaction value was .975 (95% CI = .955, .996) times more likely to be a Frequent Binge Drinker than a Nonbinging Drinker. Students that had a difference in the interaction term equivalent to one standard deviation (21 .57) were .58 times more likely to be a Frequent Binge Drinker than a Nonbinging Drinker. Dijferentiation & Perception of Best Friend at MSU '3 Alcohol Use Males. The Additive Model with perceptions of their best friend at MSU’s alcohol consumption was significant (38(4) = 25.61 , p < .001, RZCOu Sm“ = .13, Rzumm = .22) and had a successful prediction rate of 88.8%, which was 13.6% greater than having no model. Students that had one unit higher in perceptions of best fi'iend use were 1.043 (95% CI = 1.024, 1.062) times more likely to be a Frequent Binge Drinker versus a Nonbinging Drinker (see Table 80, p. 251). Students that had a higher level of perception of best friend use equivalent to one standard deviation (72.43) were 20.94 times more likely to be a Frequent Binge Drinker. The Interaction Model also was significant (33(5) = 47.43, p < .001, RZCO,‘ & 5nd1= .32, RZNW = .47) and had a successful prediction rate of 88.8%, but did not show a block improvement over the Additive Model (x20) = 2. 10, p = .148). The only significant individual predictor in the model was perception of best friend use (OR = 1.046, 95% CI = 1.026, 1.067, see Table 80, p. 251). Females. The Additive Model with perceptions of their best fiiend at MSU’s alcohol consumption was significant (38(4) = 61.52, p < .001, RZCO,‘ & 5n,“ = .36, Rznmmm = .48) and had a successful prediction rate of 82.6%, an increase of 23.9% over the Basic Model. Students that had one unit higher in perceptions of best friend use were 1.052 249 (95% CI = 1.032, 1.071) times more likely to be a Frequent Binge Drinker versus a Nonbinging Drinker (see Table 80, p. 251). Students that had a higher level of perception of best fiiend use equivalent to one standard deviation (41.48) were 7.96 times more likely to be a Frequent Binge Drinker versus a Nonbinging Drinker. The Interaction Model also was significant (38(5) = 65.35, p < .001, 122“” gm" = .38, RZNmIMC = .50) and had a successful prediction rate of 81.2%, but was only on the hinge of showing a block improvement over the Additive Model (38(1) = 3.84, p = .050). The only significant individual predictor in the model remained perception of best fiiend use, which was nearly identical in value as the Additive Model (OR = 1.057, 95% CI = 1.036, 1.079, see Table 80, p. 251). Additionally, the interaction term nearly reached a level of significance (p = .054). Difirentiation & COA Status Males. The Additive Model failed to reach a level of significance (36(4) = 3.26, p = .516, see Table 81, p. 252). The Interaction Model also failed to achieve a significance as an overall model (38(5) = 3.477, p = .627). Females. The Additive Model with COA status failed to reach a level of significance (38(4) = 5.809, p = .214, see Table 81, p. 252). The Interaction Model also failed to achieve a significance as an overall model (78(5) = 9.296, p = .098). Dzficrentiation & Expectations of Alcohol Serving as a Tension Reducer Males. The Additive Model with tension reduction expectations was significant as an overall model (38(4) = 28.32, p < .001, R2“, at s..." = .20, 182an = .30). The model had a successful prediction rate of 80.8%, which was a 5.6% improvement over having no model. Expectation of alcohol serving as a tension reducer was the only 250 Hue—o we rowmmmo WomRmmmo: 3.8335 9538 93 m8 33:2: gamma 332m 2032.508 .- womoowaosm 0». wow” magnum Cmo _ 3&8 _ mnBEom _ w _ mm _ fima _ OW _ 8.x. 9 _ w _ mm _ $35 _ OW _ 8.x. 9 momma Kama Um?” -98 93 9S HEB 9?: fun.“ -95 93 93 9mm.» 935 ram W7: -93 o.oo 93 90mm 9wa fio-R -93 9cm PE... 930 9.3m 98A >mo 9M“ 9 _ a :3 _.Nao 9mo~ TEN 9S 9 _ m 93 _.S m 93m _.umo >aa§mo 93 9NN Nd _.fic 98m Nb? -9 _ m 9 _ c 9% 93— 9meo _.Nfi mam» magma Cmo 9o; 93 ~93: _.OAu _.cma Team 93 93 ~93...» _.omn _.owm _.o-Z 58898: 30%; Um?” 9; 9mm 93 :3 9w; PM: -93 9A”. 95 993 9me No.3 wz: -98 98 :w 905 9.3; _.o-Z -98 9cm 93 93M 9%: _._ 3 >mo 93 98 um— rmec 996 N330 -9 E 9 _ 0 93 93c 9an _.Nma mom» manna—d. 68 93 93 ~93: _.oao .bwa _.omq 93 93 3.3-_._... remq _bua _.oqo UmH-W x mom“ magma :8 o on 98 Now _.omu 903 fame -98 98 um: 90.: 993 roo— ._ me A .8. 23.828. 2.... u A .3. ”so-8:3. 251 H35 3 rommmao ”$2890: 32:38 9338 an: m8 33:2: 9508 S55 Zoagamoa I 00> manna _ 2E8 _ moan—om _ w _ mm F «ii ox _ 8.x. 9 _ w _ mm # é»; _ ow _ 8.x. 9 mama Zona— Umrw -93 9mm 93 933 9%: run; -98 98 9: 9me 93A raw E": -92 98 98 90mm 93.). _.8m -93 98 PM: 930 9.3x 98a >mo 98 9 fl -\ _.g _.NAo 9ch _.38 98 9 _ m 93 I: m 93mm _.wmo >355 Kong DmH-W -98 93 9% 933 930 _.uum -98 98 93 933 9A.; _.omm ES” -98 98 9.: 93m 9me #28 -9 H u o.oo AB... 930 93m 903 g 98 93 #3 _.Nfi 9mm.» .38 98 93 9S rem 933 rum— OO> -93 98 9d 930 9 _ ma _.93 -98 93 o.oo 9on 93¢ 8.38 383030: 30% Umrw -98 93 9mm 9%; 93¢ ~03 -93 93 _.u-w 92$. 9H: flag 9": -92 9cm 9.5 903 933 fomw -93. 93 93... 93a 9.3m 9o8 >mo 98 93 #3 _.Na 9w? ~33 9S 9; 93 _.oz 933 5.3 00> -93 92 93 933 93w r80 -93 9% 92 90% 93» N8; Umrw X 00> 98 98 9B rm: 983 SEC #3 9% .98 A33 9w$ 3.88 *5 A .3. 25-839 In A .3. arc-8:3. 252 significant individual variable. Students that had a one unit higher level of expectation were 1.163 (95% C1 = 1.088, 1.244) times more likely to be a Frequent Binge Drinker versus a Nonbinging Drinker (see Table 82, p. 255). Students that had a greater perception equal to one standard deviation (8.73) were 3.74 times more likely to be Binge Drinkers. Additionally, age was a significant factor, as students that were 1 year older were 1.594 (95% CI = 1.069, 2.376) times more likely to be Frequent Binge Drinkers. Students that were two years older were 2.54 times more likely to be Frequent Binge Drinkers. The Interaction Model also was significant overall 08(5) = 29.83, p < .001, RZCO,‘ & 5...," = .21, from“, = .32) and had a successful prediction rate of 78.4%; however, it did not show a block improvement over the Additive Model (380) = 1.51, p = .219). Age (OR = 1.603, 95% CI = 1.074, 2.392) and expectations of tension reduction (OR = 1.176, 95% CI = 1.095, 1.262) remained the only significant individual predictors (see Table 82, p. 255). Females. The Additive Model with tension reduction expectations was significant as an overall model 08(4) = 55.84, p < .001, R20,“ 5...... = .33, mm = .44). The model had a successful prediction rate of 73.9%, an increase of 22.5% over not having a model. Expectations of alcohol serving as a tension reducer changed the odds of being a Frequent Binge Drinker versus a Nonbinging Drinker by 1.229 (95% CI = 1.139, 1.325) for a one unit difference in expectations (see Table 82, p. 255). By changing the difference in expectations to a unit equivalent to one standard deviation for expectations (8.81) there is a 6.14 times greater likelihood of being a Frequent Binge Drinker. 253 Additionally, BMI was a significant contributor changing the likelihood of being a Frequent Binge Drinker (OR = .765, 95% CI = .624, .937). The Interaction Model also was significant overall (38(5) = 57.9l,p < 001,122“, & gm“ = .34, from = .46) and had a successful prediction rate of 75.4%; however, it did not show a block improvement over the Additive Model (12(1) = 2.08, p = .150). Expectations of tension reduction (OR = 1.239, 95% CI = 1.143, 1.343) and BMI (OR = .776, 95% CI = .633, .953) were the only significant individual predictors (see Table 82, p. 255). Dijferentiation & Expectations of Alcohol Serving as a Social Lubricant Males. The Additive Model with social lubrication expectations was significant as an overall model (36(4) = 25.63, p < 001,18“, g 3,.“ = .19, 1(2an = .28). The model had a successful prediction rate of 80%, an increase of 4.8% over not having a model. Expectations of alcohol serving as a social lubricant (OR = 1.182, 95% CI = 1.093, 1.279) and age (OR = 1.601, 95% CI = 1.063, 2.411) were significant individual predictors of Binge Drinking category (see Table 83, p. 258). Students that had a greater expectation of social lubrication equal to one standard deviation (7.89) were 3.74 times more likely to be Frequent Binge Drinkers. Additionally, students that were two years older were 2.57 times more likely to be Frequent Binge Drinkers. The Interaction Model also was significant overall (38(5) = 26.52, p < .001, RIC... & 5.," = .19, 1?sz = .28) and had a successful prediction rate of 79.2%; however, it did not show a block improvement over the Additive Model (x2(1)= .89, p = .345). 254 Haze mm rommmno Wannammmos wnoamomam £333 93 man 33:2: 95on 388 Zoacmamoa l mxvoonmmoam om H038: Won—5:0: _ K38 _ moBEnm _emmm_€aa_ow~e§9_e_mm_€sa_ow_ ease wean Zona— cm; -93 9mm 9d 38 out: 3% -95 8M 9: 33 on: _.am we: -92 98 can 8% Sam _.So -9: o.oo an: case 3; o.oea 2a SN 9: _.ma 5.5 38 ES 98 9; 92 _.o; 3% 5% >355 Kean— UmTW 9mm 93 93 _.wna 9m: 920 9.: 93 PM: PEN 9ch era: W?= -98 90m 93 90.: 93a _._3 -93 95 92¢... 9.5m 9a: 903 >mo 93 93 MB... _.moa _.omo No.3 -95 98 9c— 9ww_ 9me _.N: Hoammoawacoaos 93 98 3.8.... _.au romm H93 95 90A N931. _.Nno #30 _.wNm 588030: zona— Uwzw 93 93 _.Ne _bwm 93A FM: 9S 9Au Na Nome 9ch 9qu W74: -99 9cm 98 90.3 9mm~ _.coA -9~m 93 9mm... 9.3a 99am 98w >mo 93 98 mm»... _.gw _.o: when -93 98 93 9mm~ 9mg _.NAo H253: ”3:030: 93 92 ~98: _.Sm _.oem HRS 93 9o... 3.3.1 _.Nwo _._Aw #qu Umzwx H958: Waggon 93 com #3 fog 90am _._wm -98 98 fee 9on 93¢ To”: _.. A .3. 25-3:59 _._... no A .3. 25-859 255 Expectations for social lubrication (OR = 1.192, 95% CI = 1.099, 1.292) and age (OR = 1.634, 95% CI = 1.080, 2.472) remained the only significant individual variables in predicting Binge Drinkers versus Nonbinging Drinkers (see Table 83, p. 258). Females. The Additive Model with social lubrication expectations was significant as an overall model (38(4) = 27.49, p < .001, 3%,, .e s...“ = .18, mm“... = .24). The model had a successful prediction rate of 68.1%, an increase of 16.7% over the not having a model. Expectations of alcohol serving as a social lubricant changed the odds of being a Frequent Binge Drinker versus a Nonbinging Drinker by 1.144 (95% CI = 1.074, 1.218) for every one unit change in perceptions (see Table 83, p. 258). Students with a difference in expectations equal to one standard deviation (7.74) were 2.82 times more likely to be a Frequent Binge Drinker. Additionally, BMI was a significant contributor in predicting Frequent Binge Drinkers versus a Nonbinging Drinkers (OR = .855, 95% CI = .731, .999). The Interaction Model also was significant overall (£6) = 28.19, p < .001, R20”, & 5...." = .19, RZNW, = .25) and had a successful prediction rate of 68.8%; however, it did not show a block improvement over the Additive Model (x20) = .70, p = .404). BMI was no longer a significant individual variable with the addition of the interaction term (see Table 83, p. 258). Expectations for social lubrication was the only significant individual predictor (OR = 1.145, 95% C1 = 1.074, 1.221) in predicting Frequent Binge Drinkers versus a Nonbinging Drinkers. The Comprehensive Model Males. The Comprehensive Model was significant according to the model chi- square (x2( 13) = 64.24, p < .001, RIC“ & 3m" = .40, Rznmmm = .60). The model had an 256 88.8% success rate in predicting the correct binge drinking classifications, which was a 13.6% improvement over having no model. Age (OR = 2.431) and perceptions of best friend use (OR = 1.040) were the only individual variables to significantly improve the odds of being classified as a Frequent Binge Drinker versus a Nonbinging Drinker (see Table 84, p. 259). Females. The Comprehensive Model was significant according to the model chi- square (x203) = 100.54, p < .001, RIC... & 5,... = .52, hm...“ = .69) and showed a significant improvement over the Basic Model (3800) = 94.73, p < .001). The model . illustrated an 85.5% success rate in predicting the correct drinking classifications, an improvement of 26.8% over the Basic Model. Perceptions of best fiiend use (OR = 1.050) and expectations of tension reduction (OR = 1.251) were the only individual variables to significantly improve the odds of being classified as a Frequent Binge Drinker versus a Nonbinging Drinker (see Table 84, p. 259). Frequent Binge Drinkers versus Occasional Binge Drinkers Differentiation and binge drinking Males. The Basic Model was not significant according to the model chi-square (78(3) = 6.83, p = .08, see Table 85, p. 261). Females. The Basic Model was not significant according to the model chi-square (£6) = 4.588, p = .205). The model had a 52.7% success rate in predicting the correct drinking classifications which was 1.3% worse than having no model. Additionally, there were no significant individual predictors for the sample, although BMI approached significance (p = .054) (see Table 85, p. 261). 257 Haze. 8 homage Womaommmo: 32:38 mean-:5 9m: :3 3383 agmflmwgm 283808 I 9:88:03 om moan: r3185: _ :38 _ moafiow _ e :3 sea _ow = «use P e I: 55 _9: 8.89 momma Zona— UmH-W -98 98 9.8 93.. flew: _.MNA -95 98 9: 9me 93A _.omm wz: -98 98 9mm 98“ meg _.o-a -98 98 PB... 9on 9.3x 9o: .98 9mm 93 :3 _.NS :88 _QNN 98 93 98 _.SM 9.3m 8% >833 Zena Um?” 93 9.: 93 :3 :88 Na: 93 9mm _.8 _.m8 9qu 8.33 mg: -98 98 9 3 934 = 88 :8 -9 _ a 98 9mg. 98m 98. 980 .98 93 9B 93... :2: __=8 NA: -92 93 93 98a 93a 83 meow». reggae: 9: 98 3.8.1. :8 = :8 _.Nqo 98 98 35.... It. :8; _.NE 58303:: Kong Ume 98 93 9mm :8. 1.88 98m 93 98 :5 88 9:8 988 ES: -98 98 98 90.8 :88 :80 -9 _ m 98 8.9 980 98m 298 >8 9% 92 9.2... _.85 _n :86 PBS -93 93 98 98w 9:0 8% mega reggae: 9; 98 3.3.... :8 _M :8 _.Nou 93 98 3.8: :3 _.ofi _.NB Ume x mega reggae: 98 98 o.oo :89 388 :8 -98 98 98 980 980 :80 .3: A .8. 3.5-8:3. _._... .u A .3. "So-8:3. 258 885 8 rommmmo wnmammmo: 32:38 mama—:8 3m» :8 33:03 9:83 «638 20:28.5 I noavnoroamE Zona— Kanm «2:28 w mm «$8 0% 88 Q 3 mm $35 0w 8.8 Q Om?» 9.8 98 98 _.83 988 o.ooo -9 _ m 98 98 98c 983 w. _ 8 ES— -9 _ o 9 3 98 980 933 _._8 -98 9 I 8.3 9m _ o 93— m _.83 >mo 98 98 3.8.... N8. _.83 P83 -9 _ 3 98 98 988 938 #88 Km: 88888 93 93 93 #8” 908 _.98 -93 93 98 908 933 _.Sa Umrw x ch 880308 98 98 98 .98 98~ _.8m -98 98 N3 98c 98.. _.o8 8% 1:33 98 9S _ _.No: _.8c 2: 3 8.88 98 93 3.8.. _._ _.8o :88 _.83 Umzw x mam: $.53 98 98 _.8 _.88 90% _.8o -98 98 98 98c 98¢ _.88 00> L .3 93 8.8 98m 98m #58 -98 98 98 93.3 9 _ 8 93¢ Um?” x 00> _.8 _.8 _.3 8.80 988 8.98 8.8 _.8 _.8 .88 988 88.83 .358: ”@988: 98 98 8.3 _.o8 98m _.8o 98 98 8.8.1. #8. _._ _ _ r88 Um?” x .338: 833:: -98 9: 98 930 938 _.NNA -98 9: _.8 98m 933 :8 won?— rccnomao: 9: 98 8.8 _._ E 988 _.N8 -98 98 98 908 983 _.omm Um?” x moan. r5188: 93 93 93 _._8 98~ _.88 93 98 98 _._8 983 .88 _._ A .8. 2535. _._. u A .2. Eoéi. 259 F our- factors of the DSI-R with binge drinking Males. The overall four-factor model was significant (x2(6) = 15.82, p = .015, RICOx a 3m" = .10, Rznmm = .13). The model had the same successful prediction rate of 63%, a 2% improvement over having no model. The “1” Position subscale was a significant predictor of binge drinking categories. Students that had a one unit higher “I” Position score were .488 (95% CI = .268, .889) times more likely to be Frequent Binge Drinkers versus Occasional Binge Drinkers (see Table 86, p. 261). Additionally, Emotional Cut-off subscale approached significance (p = .056). Females. As was the case in the Basic Model, the overall four-factor model failed to reach a level of significance (12(6) = 9.05, p = .171, see Table 86, p. 261). Dflrentiation & perception of average MSU student’s alcohol use Males. The Additive Model was significant as an overall model (38(4) = 11.031, p = .026, RZCOX a 5m“ = .069, Rzumm = .094). The model had a successful prediction rate of 62.3%, an improvement of 1.3% over having no model. Perceptions of MSU student use was the only significant individual predictor. Although perception of MSU student use approached significance (p = .053), none of the individual predictors significantly predicted being more likely to be Frequent Binge Drinkers than Nonbinging Drinkers (see Table 87, p. 263). 260 Hugo mu Famine Womaommmos vanamozsm mama—.8 9»: m8 man—:63 952$ 82% 0888:». Eamon“ I 93.23225: 3E8 mmaaom w mm (Sam OW 3.x. 9 w mm gig OW 3.x. Q 02% -93 93 mum out obmu fog o.oo 9.3 9.3 room oboe f2: ESH .93 93 PS 902 cud _.omm -9: o.om P: chow 9qu _.oow >mo PNA 93 New _.Nuu 903 _.Sm .93 0.3 93 ob: 93o rug .6 A .3. 23-6339 .1. E A .3. go-§_oa. Hugo mo rename Womaowmmon mammomnm wagon—Hm 33 m8 «Bacon. wmamonm mo obs o. ~ u #3 _.Nmu o.ouo _.omw .98 o. H A 93 935 obs _.Nwo _._ h A .3. go-§_oa. I. .u A .3. 25.659 261 The Interaction Model was significant as an overall model (x-2(5) = 14.96, p = .011, R2“, & s..." = .09, mm“... = .13), had a successful prediction rate of 64.9%, and showed a significant block improvement over the Additive Model (360) = 3.93, p = .047). None of the individual predictors significantly predicted binge drinking category, although the DSI-R (p = .051), perceptions of MSU use (p = .054), and the interaction variable (p=.061) each approached significance (see Table 87, p. 263). Females. The Additive Model with perceptions of the average MSU student’s alcohol consumption was not significant as an overall model (12(4) = 4.61 , p = .330, see Table 87, p. 263). The Interaction Model also was not significant as an overall model (38(5) = 6.280, p = .280). Difi'erentiation & Perception of Best Friend at MSU ’3 Alcohol Use Males. The Additive Model with perceptions of their best friend at MSU’s alcohol consumption was significant (752(4) = 33.27, p < .001, Ric... a 3,... = .19, alum“, = .26) I and had a successful prediction rate of 71.4%, which was 10.4% greater than having no model. Students that had one unit higher in perceptions of best friend use were 1.018 (95% CI = 1.010, 1.027) times more likely to be a Frequent Binge Drinker versus an Occasional Binge Drinker (see Table 88, p. 265). Students that had a higher level of perception of best friend use equivalent to one standard deviation (72.43) were 3.68 times more likely to be a Frequent Binge Drinker. 262 flew—n mu homage Womuommmoa 32:35 maaoam 2:: m8 125:2: Eamon. <32... 0885:». :3:QO I 338295 0». Km: manna Ema _ Kaom _ moan—om _6 _mm _ 2% _ 9: 8:9 _ 6 _mm_€sa_ 9: 3x9 momma Zeno: 63.” -98 93 whom 9mg 9N3 _.oao 98 93 93 _.ocm oboe _.03 ES: -92 93 93 903 okuu _.omw -9: 9g 93 9mg 9qu room >mo 9?: 9: New _.Nuu 93a _.Sm .98 93 93 993 92o _.Nfi >95?” Zona— Umzw -9% 9.3 no.3 9am 926 _.ooa 9S 9uo 9: 5:3 930 No3 wz: -93 93 _.5 93m 933 50$ -95 90a um: 93» 9.3a room >mo 9~A 93 who _.N-N-N ohm-o. _.mow -93 9: 9: 993 93¢ _.wa ZmC Cma 93 93 9.3 _.3o _.ooo _.omo o.oo o.oo 98 :93 98M _.ooo 38330: gono— UmZN -odw 93 93 930 9wa _.ooN 9: 9: 98 3.30 93m NZ: ES: -93 93 _.qo 98m 993 _.owa -9: 9cm 93 9mg 9.3m _.oom >mo ohm 9G 98 :boo 93m 3.20 -98 93 93 90% 9w: 3ng ZmCCmo 93 93 9.: 3.3: _.ooo _.oNN o.oo o.oo 98 _.ooo 98: _.ooc UmTWX ch Cmo -98 93 9mm 93c 90% _.o3 -93 93 _.3 90% 93M _.ooa .5 A .8. 2.0-8:09 uuwA .3. ”so-fizoa. 263 The Interaction Model also was significant (38(5) = 33.87, p < .001, R20,” 3.,“ = .20, [gum-km = .27) and had a successful prediction rate of 70.1%, but did not show a block improvement over the Additive Model (380) = .60, p = .438). The only significant individual predictor in the model was perception of best friend use (OR = 1.019, 95% C1 = 1.010, 1.028, see Table 88, p. 265). Females. The Additive Model with perceptions of their best fi'iend at MSU’s alcohol consumption was significant (x2(4) = 22.36, p < .001, R2“,- at 5....“ = .14, RZNaaelkerke = .19) and had a successful prediction rate of 64.4%, an increase of 11.7% over the Basic Model. Students that had one unit higher in perceptions of best friend use were 1.018 (95% CI = 1.009, 1.027) times more likely to be Frequent versus Occasional Binge Drinkers (see Table 88, p. 265). Students that had a higher level of perception of best fi'iend use equivalent to one standard deviation (41.48) were 2.11 times more likely to be a Frequent versus Occasional Binge Drinkers. Additionally, BMI was a significant contributor in predicting drinking categories, as subjects BMI values increased they were less likely to be Frequent Drinkers (OR = .866, 95% CI = .762, .978). The Interaction Model also was significant (x2(5) = 22.69, p < .001, R20,“ 5m“ = .14, [(21-1.8.“,ch = .19) and had a successful prediction rate of 63.7%, but did not show a block improvement over the Additive Model (380) = .33, p = .565). The only significant individual predictors in the model remained perception of best friend use (OR = 1.019, 95% CI = 1.009, 1.029) and BMI (.866, 95% CI = .762, .983, see Table 88, p. 265). 264 Hugo mm rommmao ”3838: 32:33 mama-=8 :5» >3 33:2: 95mg 538 0885:». Esme-m I $088303 om mam, magma Cmo Zaam # moan—om s _ mm _ 55 _ ow _ 8.x. 9 _ s _ mm _ é»: _ ow a 8: Q mum? Zena Umzw o.oo 98 9mm 9%.» 923 _.oao o.oo o.oo 9 H o room o.ooo _.ow: ES: -93 93 93 902 o.od _.omm -9: o.oo 9.: o.oon 93A _.ooN >mo 9?: 93 Now _.Nd o.ooo _.oqo o.oo 93 93 998 92¢ _.Nfi 25:76 Kean: Ume 9% 93 93 ohom o.ooo :oo obo 93 93 Ewen o.ooo 93m :35 o.oo o.oo Poo 92m 93o :93 -93 o.oo who. 93: 9.3» 93m >mo ohm o. _ a mom flown o.ooo _.m _ A -93 o. _ m 98 o.ooo o.ooo _.Nao mom” magma Cmo o.om o.oo Sou: _.o; _.o_o rowq 98 93 3:33.. _.o_ m #80 _.owu 583020: Zoom. Ume -9: 9.3 _.Ao 99% onto _.u; 9: 9.3 9.5 _.Nwm 93o Noon wz: o.oo o.oo _.oo o.omw 92 m 9ko -93 o.oo are: .. o.ooo th o.ooo >mo 98 o. _ o 93 _.u :o o.oo: _.m _ o -9ow o. _ m obo 93o o.ooo _.NAo wow" magma can 98 9o: 3.“: 3.. _.oE _.o_o _.owm o.om 9o: _93: _.o_o _.ooo 980 Um?” X wow" magma can 93 93 o.oo _.oou o.ooo rowu o.oo 9o: 93 :.ooA o.ooo _.omo _._ me A .8. 25.8%? 1.5 A .or Rio-8:09 265 Dzfllerentiation & COA Status Males. The Additive Model with COA status failed to reach a level of significance (36(4) = 7.30, p = .121, see Table 89, p. 268). The Interaction Model also failed to achieve a significance as an overall model (£6) = 7.309, p = .199). Females. The Additive Model with COA status failed to reach a level of significance (x2(4) = 5.65, p = .227, see Table 89, p. 268). The Interaction Model was significant as an overall model 08(5) = 11.19, p = .05, RZCM 3,... = .07, Rzmm = .10). The model had a successful prediction rate of 52.7%, which was an improvement of 1.3% over having no model. Students with a one unit higher interaction value were 6.773 (95% CI = 1.209, 37.949) times more likely to be a Frequent Binge Drinker than an Occasional Binge Drinker (see Table 89, p. 268, see Figure 7, p. 74 for interaction effect). Dijfirentiation & Expectations of Alcohol Serving as a Tension Reducer Males. The Additive Model with tension reduction expectations was significant as an overall model 08(4) = 15.09, p = .005, R2“,- & 3....- = .09, RIM-m = .13). The model had a successful prediction rate of 62.3%, which was a 1.3% improvement over having no model. Expectation of alcohol serving as a tension reducer was the only significant individual variable. Students that had a one unit higher level of expectation were 1.073 (95% CI = 1.021, 1.127) times more likely to be a Frequent versus an Occasional Binge Drinkers (see Table 90, p. 269). Students that had a greater perception equal to one standard deviation (8.73) were 1.84 times more likely to be Frequent Binge Drinkers. Additionally, age was a significant factor, as students that were 1 year older were 1.338 266 (95% CI = 1.001, 1.787) times more likely to be Frequent Binge Drinkers. Students that were two years older were 1.79 times more likely to be Frequent Binge Drinkers. ' The Interaction Model also was significant overall (x2(5) = 16.62, p = .005, R20,,( & 30¢" = .10, Rznmnmkc = .14) and had a successfill prediction rate of 61 .7%; however, it did not show a block improvement over the Additive Model (x20) = 1.54, p = .215). Age (OR = 1.377, 95% CI = 1.024, 1.852) and expectations of tension reduction (OR = 1.074, 95% CI = 1.022, 1.128) remained the only significant individual predictors (see Table 90, p. 269). Females. The Additive Model with tension reduction expectations was significant as an overall model (36(4) = 15.89, p = .003, R20,“ 5,... = .10, £2an = .14). The model had a successful prediction rate of 57.5%, an increase of 6. 1% over not having a model. Expectations of alcohol serving as a tension reducer changed the odds of being a Frequent Binge Drinker versus an Occasional Binge Drinker by 1.096 (95% CI = 1.035, 1.160) for a one unit difference in expectations (see Table 90, p. 269). By changing the difference in expectations to a unit equivalent to one standard deviation for expectations (8.81) there is a 2.23 times greater likelihood of being a Frequent Binge Drinker. The Interaction Model also was significant overall (36(5) = 15.97, p = .007, RICO. & 3n..." = .19, Rznmm = .27) and had a successful prediction rate of 59.6%; however, it did not show a block improvement over the Additive Model (x20) = .08, p = .778). Expectations of tension reduction remained the only significant individual predictor (OR = 1.095, 95% CI = 1.035, 1.160, see Table 90, p. 269). 267 o.oo—a we rommmoo ”£838: 3.8335 maaoam 5m» >8 33:2: Eamon 825 008925— wmsmoa l 00> mBEm _ 3E8 _ 123—om L a :m fléea _ ow _ 8.x. 2 _ e _ mm _ 2% _ ow _ 3.x. 9 momma Zona— UmTW o.oo o.oo o.oo out ohwu #vo o.oo o.oo o. _ o o.ooo o.ooo o.oo— Eé -o.oA o.om o.oo o.ooo o.ooo o.omm -o._ _ o.oo Pd o.oom o.ooo _.oou >mo ob... o. H a New _.Nd o.ooo _.ouo o.oo o. _ A o. o o 903 o..2o ENS >353 Zona— UmEN o.oo o.oo 93 ohm— o.Nco _.og o.oo o.oo o.oo _.omu o.oo_ _.ooo wzo o.oo o.om o.oo o.oo.“ o.od o.oow -o.: o.oo who o.ooo. o.ooo _.ooc .9? ob.“ o. 3 NS _.Nqu o.oom _.oqm -o.oq 9: ob.“ o.ooo ohom _.NK 00> -o.w_ o.oo o.oo o.ooo o.ooo TEN ohm ohm o.om _.mao o.ooo Pin 588020: gono— Umzw -93 o.oo NE om: obom _.o _N -obu o.oo 93 o.ooo. o.oom _.Aoo w?= -o.oA 93 ohm o.oo; 93m _.oou -o._~ o.oo who o.oo.» o.ooo foo— >m@ on» o. _ A New _.Nd o.oou _.oqo o.oo o._ m ohm o.ono o.ooo _.Nwo 00> oh: ohm 93 9qu o.ooo _.qmm ohm o.oo ohm _.A _ w ouqo 9A3 szw x 00> o.oq o.oo o.oo _.oom oboo 95$ _.3 o.oo Pd... 99d _.Noo 3.vo n to A .8. 23.8%? 3h A or 2.5-8:.”9 268 Hugo co rommmno Wowammmo: 38323 £888 5:: >3 man—8:» mgmoa <29; Geomagm— 259m l mxuooflmaoa ow H038: W888: _ 3&8 _ maBEom _ a o mm _ éoa _ o: _ 8&9 _ o 4 mm _ fins 8 o: _ 8:9 wmmmo :81 Uwzw o.oo o.oo o.oo out ohoq :vo o.oo o.oo o. A o _.oon o.ooo :3: BE -o.oA o.om o.oo. o.ofl o.ooo _.omo -o.: o.oo on: o.oow o.ooA _.ooN >mo o.~A o. A New _.Nd o.ooo o.ooo o.oo o. _ A o. _ o o.oAm o..:o A.NA.\ >835 382 Ume -obm ohm ohm ohwm o.ooo A.Awo o.oo ohm who _.dq o.omu Pmoo wz: -o.oA o.ou o.AA o.ooo o.od _.ooo -o._ _ o.oo Non o.ooo o.ooo _.S u >mo obo o.Am o.oo... o.ooo _.oo_ _.qmq .98 93 93 o.oum on“; _.Nd #38: W885: o.oq o.oo $3.1. o.ooo _.oB :3 o.oo 98 com: _.ooo room :3 5838:: 38¢. Ume .93 o.om o.oo o.ooo .wmA _.A_o o.oo o.oo NuA ooNA o.oun o.ooo wz: -o.oA 93 9% o.ooo o.ooo _.oon -o. _ o o.oo ~.oA o.ooo o.ooo _.oB >mo ohm o._u A.Ao... $3 _.oNA _.omn -o.oA o._m o.oo 99% oh: _.NE Adamo: ”8:80: o.oq o.ow Soon... _.oQA _.oun _._No o.oo 98 who: _.oou _.ouu Zoo Ume x H038: W883: o.oo 93 _.Au 9vo o.ooo _.ouA .93 93 o.oo o.ooo o.co_ _.omn . n A .8. 398:8. _._. a A .3. 253.8. 269 Dzfikrentiation & Expectations of A lcohol Serving as a Social Lubricant Males. The Additive Model with social lubrication expectations was significant as an overall model (38(4) = 9.53, p = .049, 82¢... a 5,... = .06, 82%“, = .08). The model had a successful prediction rate of 60.4%, an increase of .6% over not having a model. Although the model was significant, none of the individual variables were significant in the prediction of binge drinking category (see Table 91, p. 271). The Interaction Model failed to reach a level of significance (38(5) = 9.54, p = .089, see Table 91, p. 271). Females. The Additive Model with social lubrication expectations was significant as an overall model (36(4) = 12.18, p = .016, 13¢,” 5,... = .08, 112an = .11). The model had a successful prediction rate of 59.6%, an increase of 8.2% over not having a model. Expectations of alcohol serving as a social lubricant changed the odds of being a Frequent Binge Drinker versus an Occasional Binge Drinker by 1.078 (95% CI = 1.020, 1.140) for every one unit change in perceptions (see Table 91, p. 271). Students with a difference in expectations equal to one standard deviation (7.74) were 1.79 times more likely to be a Frequent Binge Drinker. The Interaction Model also was significant overall (36(5) = 12.18, p = .032, 18%,, a 5...." = .08, thi..g.m.ke = .11) and had a successful prediction rate of 71.8%; however, it did not show a block improvement over the Additive Model (x20) = .00, p = .982). Expectations for social lubrication remained the only significant individual predictor (OR = 1.078, 95% CI = 1.020, 1.140) in predicting Frequent versus Occasional Binge Drinkers (see Table 91, p. 271). 270 Hugo 3 homage Woman-ammo: 32:38 930:8 92 >8 mama—ma Ego; 58% 088mg:— wm:mo~m I mxnooBmoPa ow mega rccnommo: — 3&8 _ mafia—am _ e _ mm _éa: o: _ moan: _ a _ mm. as; _ ox _ 392: mean Kong Um?” -98 98 8.8 9qu 9mm: _.vo 98 93 98 room 926 be“: male: -98 98 93 903 938 98a -9: 98 8.: 98» 93A _.oou go 9N8. 9: 98 _.N8 98a _.a-R -98 9: 93 903 9.2m _.NS >aaEmo 93 93 93 _.uow 98m _.qwm -98 93 98 93M 98¢ _.Nom meow»: rccnomao: 99A 98 N65 _.oAu 903 _._8 98 98 98...... _.o-Nm _.ouc _._Ao 38898: Zona— UmH-W -93 98 _.wh 93g 98a fuNA 9% 9w» _.3 _.3o 93w “:8 W73 -98 98 93 93m 933 _.o-Nw -95 o.oo 8.3 9%: 9qu _.cow >Mn 93 9; 8.3 _.woo 93A 9qu -98 9G 98 93M 9.3g _.Nom moan—“15183:: 98 98 N8 2.3 993 ram 98 98 $81.. _.oqm _.owc _.Eo Umfi-WX mega rcvaowmo: coo 98 9o— ?ook— 90: _._oo 98 98 o.oo _.oS 9on Tom: e h A .8. 25.838. _._... .u A .2. go-Szoa. 27l The Comprehensive Model Males. The Comprehensive Model was significant according to the model chi- square (38(13) = 46.1 1, p < .001, R20,” 3...,- = .26, mm...“ = .35). The model illustrated a 73.4% success rate in predicting the correct drinking classifications, a 12.4% improvement over having no model. Age (OR = 1.472), perceptions of best friend use (OR = 1.018), and the differentiation X expectations of tension reduction interaction (OR = .826, see Figure 8, p. 77, for interaction) were the only individual variables to significantly improve the odds of being classified as a Frequent Binge Drinker versus an Occasional Binge Drinker (see Table 92, p. 273). Females. The Comprehensive Model was significant according to the model chi- . square (x203) = 39.46, p < .001, R2c°x ,- 3...- = .24, 12sz = .32) and showed a significant improvement over the Basic Model (x2(10)= 34.88, p < .001). The model had a 69.2% success rate in predicting the correct drinking classifications, an improvement of 16.5% over the Basic Model. Perceptions of best friend use (OR = 1.019) and expectations of tension reduction (OR = 1.081) were the only individual variables to significantly improve the odds of being classified as a Frequent versus an Occasional Binge Drinker (see Table 92, p. 273). 272 Had—o 0N homage ”om—6&8: 3.35:8 magma 9R Ed 3.00:2: Esau; 388 08333. 9803 I 02:96:03?» gono— Zaom man—Ema 8 mm £35 0% 8.x. 9 8 mm 83m OW 8.x. Q 69% -98 98 _.8 98w 98¢ :88 9mm 93 _.8 .88 9mm.“ 8.88 wz: -98 98 Hum 908 9a _ m _.98 -9 8 98 u .8 988 9.88 _.o . o >m 980 9; 8.8... _.SN _.8. ~88 -98 93 93 908 9.88 _.80 Km: 02.80.83 98 93 98— ro8 900. :03 98 93 95 900m 908 .88 Um?” X ch 0280308 -98 93 N00 908 908 :08 -92 93 98 900m 980 for. mam" manna 98 9S 5&0: _.o; _.8m _.8m 98 93 3.3.1. 2:0 _.8m #8— Umzw X mam. 3.5:. 98 92 8.3. _.c; 900m _.80 98 9o— 9: :88 908 _.ouu 00> -93 98 9d 998 980 _.88 -9 5 9m. 98 98— 988 N88 62% X 00> 9N0 _.8 98 #88 93c 2.38 _.8 _.8 N8 986 988 8.88 dogma: X8230: 98 98 93 78¢ 908 :3 98 98 8.3... _.2: _.8m :8 0m?” X H955: ”8:30: .93 98 .30... 98¢ 988 908 -98 98 98 908 98w _._ _N moomfi 551832. -98 98 98 98m 908 :08 98 98 _._u _.80 908 _._ _m 09% X mafia r5183: 93 9C0 Pam _._m0 908 _.8m 98 98 9G _.80 9m0_ :8 *5 A .8. "so-3.0m. _._... E A .3. 25-889 273