. _ 4 1 .34. V iaafif....amfi? ,4. . a424,...» :. «L. . . 4 . r. J. ‘2 .— auiygz. x. , . , . a 31". v”. ._‘d.. 1 . 2... s23 usfluwfian .. ,2. . I”? W 1...: ‘ 2. .uflunwna.’ r . .x... 33.. a; .3 .. . . J? .u.:... .9 Lit. ... .3: .1. R . s4 «IQ. . flaw? «an “awn...“umn .r 3L ; . . “Jami“..fi :5”! (Lu! ‘1 1:“ € 1 Winn“ h: v .. a: t. 2?! Pt. 3‘... 1h... :tisl; 13031.23 . 1.. .himrut. 1513;? A .. .L . ‘ 15...". ‘ .15.... .c. . .. THESIS 5 l LIBRARIES J o 0 b MICHIGAN STATE UNIVERSITY EAST LANSING, MICH 48824-1048. This is to certify that the dissertation entitled Postdivorce Parental Relationships and Well-being among African American, Hispanic, and Caucasian College Students presented by Janet M. Kinney has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for the PhD. degree in Counseling Psychology A Queue ALMA, PA ( b . (Major Professor’s Signature 08/zs/Q5 Date MSU is an Affirmative Action/Equal Opportunity Institution ’ .7 o —- -‘v —- ‘ ~— g, 7‘ - v ‘7 '— 9 - v 4’ fl *' v '- ' Fyrgv—H-‘ -“W QM“ fiv—K‘ PLACE IN REfURN BOX to remove this checkout from your record. TO AVOID FINES return on or before date due. MAY BE RECALLED with earlier due date if requested. DATE DUE DATE DUE DATE DUE 2/05 chlFlC/DateOuoindd—pds POSTDIVORCE PARENTAL RELATIONSHIPS AND WELL-BEING AMONG AFRICAN AMERICAN, HISPANIC, AND CAUCASIAN COLLEGE STUDENTS By Janet M. Kinney A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Counseling, Educational Psychology and Special Education 2005 ABSTRACT POSTDIVORCE PARENTAL RELATIONSHIPS AND WELL-BEING AMONG AFRICAN AMERICAN, HISPANIC, AND CAUCASIAN COLLEGE STUDENTS By Janet M. Kinney Adolescents' relationships with parents have long been acknowledged as important contributors to psychosocial development. The purpose of this study was to examine parental attachment and interparental conflict in an effort to explain why some adolescents fare well after the divorce of their parents while others do not. Of particular interest were the separate and unique contributions of attachment style (e.g., secure, insecure) with mother (MlAtt) and maternal initiated conflict with father (MICON) to attachment style with father (PIAtt) and adolescent well-being (WB). Attachment to parents was examined by comparing MIAtt and P/Att with regard to three domains: affective quality of relationships, facilitator of independence, and source of support. Participants were 58 African American (AA), 28 Hispanic (H), and 93 Caucasian (C) 18-23 year old young adults from a Midwestern university who came from divorced families in which mothers were the custodial parent. Across the racial/ethnic groups, participants viewed their relationships with mother and father as both independent and interdependent. Path analysis of the two groups indimted a statistically significant relationship between the entire model and the outcome variable. The prsr AR"! m 957‘ proposed model accounted for a greater portion of variance in WB within the AAIH group (R2 = .79) than within the C group (R2 = .35). The findings supported the hypotheses that 1) secure MIAtt and low levels of MICON correspond with secure P/Att, 2) secure MIAtt and PIAtt resulted in higher levels of general, spiritual, and psychological WB, and 3) H and AA participants demonstrate more adaptive strategies to MICON than C participants. Significant differences between the groups are illustrated and discussed. The findings suggest that it is important to evaluate the relationship between parental attachment within the context of family process variables that may explain the parental divorce-child psychosocial adjustment relationship. Dedication This dissertation is dedicated to the Lord my God, my family and my friends, without which my dream could never have come true. Thank you for believing in me and supporting me in your own unique way. I especially recognize and appreciate my husband, Jack, and my daughters, Samantha and Sarah. I love you very much and recognize the sacrifices you have made as l have completed this journey. iv .I Tarra E Ph D unwarj I pray? SUDDC‘ Com." expene interns and pr: 90W C»: We 3M ACKNOWLEDGMENTS First, I would like to thank the members of my Dissertation Committee, J. Tawa Sina, Ph.D., Gloria Smith, Ph.D., Robert Caldwell, Ph.D., and Lee June, Ph.D., for lending their special expertise. Special thanks to Dr. Smith for her unwavering support and the stability she provided to the Counseling Psychology program. My sincere appreciation goes out to Dr. Sina for her many years of support and guidance in her roles as chairperson of my Doctoral Dissertation Committee, advisor of my doctoral program, supervisor of my practicum experience at the Counseling Center, and Training Director during my doctoral internship. Irrespective of her role, Dr. Sina has modeled integrity, genuineness, and professionalism. She encouraged me to perseverance and motivated me to grow personally and professionally. For this and much more, I thank her greatly. Next, I would like to specially thank Robert Fabiano, Ph.D. for the major role he played in my doctoral process. His support, training, and direction, in addition to his friendship, were invaluable to me. I would also like to recognize and thank Ellie Bossi, Barb Walkington, Bets Caldwell, Mary Ball, Rosalyn Davis, and Karen Brown for their support and assistance. Finally, I would like to acknowledge the years of professional and personal support provided by my doctoral classmates, Pei-chen Hsu, Anne Kubal, Sarah Raymond, Steve Pence, and Ariel Agosto. The support system and friendships we developed often served as my encourager and saving grace. UST UST' CHAC CHAF‘ UTER CHAP‘ MET-H, CHAP: RESUL TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES LIST OF FIGURES CHAPTER 1 Introduction Attachment Theory Influence of Attachment on Separated/Divorced Families Relationship Between Maternal and Paternal Attachment Maternal Conflict with Father Race, Separation/Divorce, and Attachment Age and Attachment Parental Attachment and Adolescent Well-being Research Questions Summary and Hypotheses CHAPTER 2 LITERATURE REVIEW Postdivorce Father Involvement and Father-Child Relationships Mother-child, Father-child, and Mother-Father Relationships Parental Conflict Attachment Theory Age and Attachment Race/Ethnicity and Attachment Summary CHAPTER 3 METHOD Participants Measures Design Data-Gathering Procedures Data Analysis CHAPTER 4 RESULTS Preliminary Analyses Correlational Analyses Path Analyses viii A—l—i—l comahommoihw-s 20 20 22 24 26 29 33 38 4O 4O 4O 50 51 52 59 59 59 75 CHAPTER 5 DISCUSSION 94 Parental Attachment, Parental Conflict, and Adolescent Well-being 94 Limitations 105 Relevance to Practice 107 Future Research 109 APPENDICES APPENDIX A Forms and Demographic lnforrnation 113 APPENDIX B African American & Hispanic Group Path Analysis 1 120 APPENDIX C Caucasian Group Path Analysis 1 134 APPENDIX D Caucasian Group Path Analysis 2 142 APPENDIX E African American 8. Hispanic Group Path Analysis 2 157 REFERENCES 186 vii Tatse Table Table Tat-e Table Table Table T3216 Table Tatie Table LIST OF TABLES Table 1 Correlation Coefficients of Adolescent Well-being Measures Table 2 African American and Hispanic Group Correlation Coefficients of Well-being Scores and Independent Variables Table 3 Caucasian Group Correlation Coefficients of Well-being Scores and Independent Variables Table 4 African American and Hispanic Group Correlations of Main Variables Table 5 Caucasian Group Correlations of Main Variables Table 6 African American and Hispanic Group Pearson Correlation Coefficients of Endogenous Variables and Possible Covariates Table 7 Caucasian Group Pearson Correlation Coefficients of Endogenous Variables and Possible Covariates Table 8 Pearson Correlation Coefficients African American and Hispanic Group Attachment Subscales, Well-being, and Maternal conflict with Father Table 9 Pearson Correlation Coefficients Caucasian Group Attachment Subscales, Well-being, and Maternal conflict with Father Table 10 African American and Hispanic Group Pearson Correlation Coefficients Endogenous and Exogenous Variables Used in Path 1 Table 11 African American and Hispanic Group Path 1 Observed and Reproduced Correlations Table 12 Summery for Causal Effects for African American and Hispanic Group Path 1 Table 13 Caucasian Group Pearson Correlation Coefficients Endogenous and Exogenous Variables Used in Path 1 Table 14 African American and Hispanic Group Path 1 Observed Correlations and Standardized Path Coefficients viii 59 6O 6O 67 67 72 72 75 78 78 81 Table Table Table Tatie Tame Tatle Table IBCIE Table Table Table Table Table Table 15 Pearson Correlation Coefficients Caucasian Group Endogenous 84 and Exogenous Variables Used in Path 2 Table 16 Caucasian Group Path 2 Observed and Reproduced Correlations 85 Table 17 Summery for Causal Effects for Caucasian Group Path 2 Outcome 86 Table 18 Pearson Correlation Coefficients African American and Hispanic 90 Group Endogenous and Exogenous Variables Used in Path 2 Table 19 African American and Hispanic Group Path 2 Observed and 90 Reproduced Correlations Table 20 Summery for Causal Effects for African American and Hispanic 93 Group Path 2 Table A1 Caucasian Group Descriptive Statistics for Demographic 117 lnforrnation Table A2 Caucasian Group Descriptive Statistics for Subscales and 117 Full-scale Parental Attachment Measures. Table A3 African American and Hispanic Group Descriptive Statistics 118 for Demographic Information Table A4 African American and Hispanic Group Descriptive Statistics 119 for Subscales and F uII-scale Parental Attachment Measures. Table A5 Cronbach’s Alpha 1 19 Table B African American and Hispanic Group Path 1 Decompositions 133 and Calculations of Reproduced Correlations Table D Caucasian Group Path 2 Decompositions and Calculations of 154 Reproduced Correlations Table E African American and Hispanic Group Path 2 Decompositions 180 and Calculations of Reproduced Correlations ix A F Fiver: U Flat/E U 5.3.. ,5 45“ LIST OF FIGURES Figure 1 Input Path Diagram 1: African American! and Hispanic Group Path Analysis 1 Figure 2 Input Path Diagram 2: Caucasian Group Path Analysis 1 Figure 3 Input Path Diagram 3: Caucasian Group Path Analysis 2 Figure 4 Input Path Diagram 4: African American and Hispanic Group Path Analysis 2 Figure B1 African American and Hispanic Group Stepwise Multiple Regression for Path Analysis 1 Figure 82 African American and Hispanic group Normal Probability Plot of the Regression Standardized Residuals for Path Analysis 1 Figure B3 African American and Hispanic Residuals Scatter plot for Path Analysis 1 Figure C1 Caucasian Group Stepwise Multiple Regression for Path Analysis 1 Figure C2 Caucasian group Normal Probability Plot of the Regression Standardized Residuals for Path Analysis 1 Figure C3 African American and Hispanic Residuals Scatter plot for Path Analysis 1 Figure D1 Caucasian Group Stepwise Multiple Regression for Path Analysis 2 Figure DZ Caucasian Group Normal Probability Plot of the Regression Standardized Residuals of Path Analysis 2 Figure D3 Caucasian Group Residuals Scatter plot for Path Analysis 2 Figure D4 Caucasian Group Path 2 Paternal Promotion of Independence Figure 05 Caucasian Group Path 2 Paternal Feelings of Affect Figure D6 Caucasian Group Path 2 Father Feelings of Support 75 81 91 121 131 132 135 140 141 143 153 153 156 156 156 th'e Fi§§Jl Figure E1 African American and Hispanic Group Stepwise Multiple Regression for Path Analysis 2 Figure E2 African American and Hispanic group Normal Probability Plot of the Regression Standardized Residuals for Path Analysis 1 Figure E3 African American and Hispanic Residuals Scatter plot for Path Analysis 1 Figure E4 African American and Hispanic Group Path 2 Paternal Promotion of Independence Figure E5 African American and Hispanic Group Path 2 Paternal Feelings of Affect Figure E6 African American and Hispanic Group Path 2 Paternal Feelings of Support xi 157 178 179 184 184 185 Int. Postdivorce Parental Relationships and Well-being among African American, Hispanic, and Caucasian College Students Introduction Adolescents are highly valued in our society and therefore, there is concern for their adjustment and well-being. Parental attachment and conflict merit attention in the context of adolescents' well-being post-divorce and therefore, will be the foci of this investigation. The number of adolescents with divorced parents has continued to grow over the last several decades and since 1972, one million American children every year have seen their parents divorce (US. Bureau of the Census, 2005). Some adolescents display negative behavioral and/or psychosocial effects (e.g., decrease in school performance, delinquency, depression, and difficulties with interpersonal relationships) that can be attributed to the divorce of their parents, while others do not. The factors that lead to negative effects for some adolescents are not clear, but there is a continuing effort to identify and understand this area. One plausible explanation for why some adolescents exhibit psychosocial and behavioral difficulties post- divorce may be attachment. Specifically, the types of relationship children have with their non-residential fathers. The father-child relationship in turn might be affected by the type of relationship adolescents have with their custodial mothers. Another factor may be the degree of conflict exhibited by the custodial mother toward the noncustodial father following a divorce. Parental conflict has been shown to have negative impact on psychosocial adjustment across a wide array of family structures and settings (Davies & Cummings, 1994: Erel & Burman, 1995), but has yet to be studied in the context of adolescents' perceived well- ‘7’.“ and C35: fa {'11 W96 ”55:; 362x being and adjustment after divorce among different racial groups. The percentage of those who are divorced has increased across all racial groups, yet the vast majority of research on divorce focuses on the experience of Caucasians, leaving a considerable gap in the literature. Contemporary changes in family structures and in social attitudes toward divorced families underscore the importance of studying the diverse roles fathers play in their children’s lives (Harris & Salt, 1999). Given that almost half of the children born in the last 10 years will experience the divorce of their parents (Gallagher, 1996; Hetherington, 1992), there is growing concern about the consequences of father separation on the child. Currently, 23% of American children live primarily with their mothers compared with less than 3% living primarily with their fathers (US. Census Bureau, 2001 ). Consequently, divorce research has continued to focus primarily on families in which mother has custody and father is the visiting parent. Of those children not residing with their fathers, 35% see their father at least once per week and some several times per week (Selzer, 1998). Divorce an be a prolonged process and many practitioners and researchers have focused on the consequences of this process on children’s well-being. The literature indicates that parental divorce is significantly associated with child psychosocial adjustment (for reviews, refer to Amato, 1993; Amato & Keith, 1991); however, there is substantial variability in children’s response to divorce (Amato, 1993; Jekielek, 1998; Seltzer, 1994; Whiteside & Becker, 2000). This variability has led to the investigation of family process 5533' have benNi fixhe «m4 553'. m Toma exar ates Iowa Afiat SEWI res; teem D's-r? variables that may explain the parental divorce-child psychosocial adjustment relationship. Parent-child relationships and parental conflict are among the family process variables that have been associated with the adjustment of children after parental divorce (Whiteside & Becker, 2000). Although few studies have examined family process variables to clarify the long-term association between parental divorce and psychosocial adjustment of older adolescents, father-adolescent and father-young adult relationships have been found to be significant predictors of young adult psychosocial adjustment (Summers, Forehand, An'nistead, & Tannenbaum, 1998). Missing in the research is the examination of the father-adolescent relationship in relation to 1) the mother- adolescent relationship and 2) the degree of conflict exhibited by the mother toward the father following a divorce. Attachment Theory Over the last several decades, attachment theory (Bowlby, 1969) has served as an important framework for conceptualizing the parent-child relationship. Parental attachment refers to the enduring emotional bond and feelings of security children perceive in their relationship with parents. A basic premise of this theory is that the infant’s initial experiences of attachment become cognitively represented in the form of “internal working models” of the self and others. These working models incorporate expectations about how worthy one is and how accessible and responsive others will be to providing support and protection. In regards to fathers, few studies have examined the benefits of paternal attachment in postdivorce families, and little research has diVOhf specifically explored the effects of divorce on the father-child relationship. Amato and Booth (1996) found that some young adults who experienced divorce as children show less affection for their noncustodial father than for their custodial mother. Yet, why this is the case for some young adults and not others has not been identified. We do know however that children who perceive their fathers as accepting, supportive, and trustworthy are more likely to report feeling closer to their fathers (Stevenson & Black 1995). Influences of Attachment on Separated/Divorced Families Secure attachment emerges from the supportiveness and safety of the child’s environment (Thompson, Lamb, & Estes, 1982), which is affected by the type of relationship between the parents (Das Eiden, Teti, & Corns, 1995). When the caregiving environment changes because of parental separation or divorce, the style of attachment (secure versus insecure) may change as well (Asendorpf 8: Wilpers, 2000; Belsky, Campbell, Cohn, & Moore, 1996). Within families where mothers are the custodial parent, father-child attachment style has been shown to be vulnerable to changes in the caregiving environment (Cummings & Watson-O’Reilly, 1997) and to the relationship children have with their mothers (Doyle, Markiewicz, Brendgen, Lieberman, & Voss, 2000; Hojat, 1998). The influence of mothers on children’s well-being is prevalent in the research. Within divorced families, children’s attachment classifications have been found to change in relation to changes in maternal behaviors (Vaughn, Egeland, Sroufe, & Waters, 1997) and children’s perceptions of maternal attachment, rather than paternal attachment, have been linked to psychosocial adjustment in adulthood «$.- A... at RSI; 590:1: 93's.» ”55a: (Hojat, 1998). In addition, mothers appear to have the ability to facilitate or impede the noncustodial father's involvement with his children (Braver, Wolchik, Sandler, Fogas, & Svetina, 1991), thereby encouraging or dissuading the father- child relationship. A next step in this line of inquiry was to explore how children’s attachment style with custodial mothers influenced children’s attachment style with noncustodial fathers. Relationship between Maternal and Paternal Attachment Attachment style is often viewed as stable and consistent across different relationships due to the strong influence of the internal working model (Bartholomew & Horowitz, 1991; Hazan & Shaver, 1987), however most researchers report independence in the attachment classifications for mother- child and father-child dyads (Grossman, Grossman, Huber, & Warner, 1981; Kenny, Lomax, Brabeck, & Fife, 1998; Main & Weston, 1981). In their meta- analyses of 14 studies, Van lJzendoorn and DeWolff (1997) stated, “By and large, infant attachment security does not appear to generalize substantially across relationships within the family system” (p. 607). They concluded that infant attachment security is more relationship-specific than infant-specific. However a meta-analyses of 11 studies involving 672 participants found that attachment to mother and attachment to father were interdependent (Fox, Kimmerly, & Schafer, 1991). That analysis indicated that children classified as secure to one parent are highly unlikely to be classified as insecure to the other parent and vice versa. Still other studies have found that attachment in parental relationships cannot be sufficiently captured without examining consistency acres Ase: stre iv!" U'hbt mid. 12 s: deve: pare: (first: WW3" " V \I pater across relationships as well as relationship-specific qualities. For example, Asendorpf and Wilpers (2000) found that changes in young adult attachment style reflected changes in their parental relationship style, and they also found support for cross-relationship consistency of attachment between the two parental relationships. The inconsistency of findings lent merit to further examination of the relationship between the three family dyads, i.e., the mother- child, the father-child, and the mother-father relationship. In a meta-analysis of 12 studies, Whiteside and Becker (2000) concluded that relationships children develop with their fathers cannot be viewed independently of the quality of the parental alliance. Custodial mothers serve as primary caregivers and have more direct influences on children’s everyday activities, so it was important to explore whether children’s style of maternal attachment influenced children’s style of paternal attachment. Maternal Conflict Wrth Fathers Adolescent psychosocial adjustment has been associated with supportive co-parenting, low conflict between parents, and better personal adjustment of parents, while adolescent difficulties have been associated with continued anger and disagreement between parents (Hetherington, Cox, & Cox, 1982; Wallerstein 8. Kelly, 1980). According to Kurkowski, Gordon, and Arbuthnot (1993), children from divorced families are caught in the middle of parental conflicts significantly more often and experience more stress than children from intact homes. Ahrons (1994) found that fifty percent of white, middle-class divorced couples engage in bitter, open conflict and only twelve percent were able to create pleasant, Iow- Cox. sac; Tall: disc: invc: conflict relationships after the divorce. Interparental conflict has been identified consistently as a major correlate of behavior problems in children across a wide array of family structures and settings (for reviews Davies 8. Cummings, 1994; Erel & Burman, 1995). A meta-analysis of 57 studies focusing on the style of paternal involvement indicated that enduring emotional bonds are more strongly associated with positive child outcomes than with frequency of contact (Amato & Gilbreth, 1999). However, without spending time with their children, it is less likely that fathers will have the opportunity to develop and/or maintain a quality relationship with their children. The level of father involvement after divorce can be substantially affected by the co-parental relationship (Hetherington, Cox, & Cox, 1982; McKenry, Price, F ine, & Servovich, 1992). Conflict with the former spouse is one of the major barriers to maintaining father involvement (lhinger- Tallman, Pasley, 8. Buehler, 1993). Interestingly, Whiteside and Becker (2000) in their meta-analysis discovered the impact of cooperation between parents and the level of father’s involvement with children were different for the mother-child relationship than for the father-child relationship. Whereas cooperative parenting facilitated the father-child relationship through greater frequency of father visits with their children and higher-quality relationships with children, cooperative parenting, hostility between parents, and greater frequency of father visits were negatively related to the mother-child relationship style. Fathers that were less collaborative were associated with mothers that exhibited more warmth and support of children (Whiteside & Becker, 2000). Therefore, even though high conflict between pa'e A" ,. CC'Vb mct.‘ 355-: stjte Over 1 736: If”. 'a parents interferes with the mother-child and father-child relationships, cooperative parenting and high paternal involvement may correspond with mothers having less time with their children and feeling less validated in their role as a mother. Thus, in addition to investigating the interdependence of the mother-child and father-child attachment style, it is important to evaluate the association between maternal conflict with fathers and father-child attachment style. If mothers benefit from less father involvement and poor father-child relationships, mothers may be consciously or subconsciously sabotaging their children’s-relationship with their fathers. In this study, the construct of maternal conflict with father will be measured by examining adolescent perceptions of maternal hostility toward father, maternal facilitation] interference of paternal involvement, and cooperative parenting behaviors of mothers. Race, Separation/Divorce, and Attachment A significant limitation of the literature on parental separation/divorce and adolescent adjustment is its exclusive focus on the experience of Caucasian, middle class children and adolescents (Hines, 1997; Ogbu, 1988). The percentage of those who are separated or divorced has increased across all racial groups. Census Bureau statistics show that in 2004, 15.5% of African Americans (4.3 and 11.2% respectively), 11.3% of Caucasians (1.7 and 9.6% respectively), and 10.6% of Hispanics (3.5 and 7.1% respectively) age 18 and over were separated or divorced (US. Bureau of the Census, 2005). African American and Hispanic children are more likely than Caucasian children to be living with one parent; 57% for African American children, 32% for Hispanic chi}: T“ (L! a df‘iOl eTICC Aft: . a... that "Bar. Set: Date 31kg, 605:6 children, and 22% for White children in 1996 (US. Bureau of the Census, 1996). Thirty-seven percent of these one-parent households are the result of parental divorce (US. Bureau of the Census, 1996). It is imperative that researchers broaden their perspective in this area to encompass race and ethnicity. Separation and divorce rates are higher for African American families, but we know little about the effects of divorce and the influence of the parent-adolescent relationship on African American youth and the literature contains virtually no information on the effects of divorce on Hispanic youth (Hines, 1997). Although few studies have examined race/ethnicity to clarify the long-term association between parental divorce and psychosocial adjustment of older adolescents, those that have run analyses differentiating between African American and Caucasian youth report inconsistent results. Some researchers have reported no significant differences between African American and Caucasian adolescents’ attachment style (Bakermans-Kranenburg, Van lJendoom, & Kroonenberg, 2004; Mason, 2005; Schlack, 2003; Van IJzenboorn & Krooneneberg, 1988) or their response to parental divorce (Rice, Cunningham, & Young, 1997: Veneziano & Rohner, 1998; Zimmerman, Salem, & Maton, 1995), while others have reported significant differences (Dombusch, Carlsmith, Bushwall, Ritter, Leiderman, Hastorf, & Gross, 1985; Famworth, 1984; Hanson, 1999; Lopez, Melendez, & Rice, 2000). A culturally infused understanding of attachment leads to the acknowledgement that parenting practices are embedded within the cultural context of a parent-child relationship and greater attachment-related autonomy or proximity is encouraged as an element of the working model and attachment style of that culture (Van lJzendoorn & Sagi, 1999). For example, the pattern of establishing extended families appears to be an adaptive strategy common to African American and Hispanic individuals (Harrison, Wilson, Pine, Chan, 8: Buriel, 1990). Three times as many African American children under the age of 18 live with their grandparents, as do Caucasian Americans (US. Bureau of the Census, 1996). Kenny and Perez (1996) found that 27% of minority students identified their primary attachment figure as someone other than a parent. A caregiving environment that consists of a network of more or less stable relationships between the individual and several caregivers may be optimal (Donley, 1993) and may shelter the adolescent from some of the negative consequences associated with parental divorce, including insecure attachment style and parental conflict. In this study, racial/ethnic differences in postdivorce parental relationships and well-being among African American, Hispanic, and Caucasian adolescents were explored. Age and Attachment The role of attachment relationships in promoting well-being during late adolescence (18-23 year olds), has been receiving increased attention (Woodward, Pergusson, 8. Belsky, 2000). In an attempt to identify predictors of college adjustment and well-being among late-adolescent students, researchers have studied the influence of secure parental attachment (Arrnsden & Greenberg, 1987; Brack, Gay, & Matheny, 1993; Bradford & Lyddon, 1993; Kenny, 1987; Kenny & Donaldson, 1991; Larose and Boivin, 1998; Mattanah, 10 Hancock, & Brand, 2004; Rice, Fitzgerald, Whaley, 8: Gibbs, 1995; Vrvona, 2000). Parental figures serve as a secure base from which adolescents explore and as a source of comfort in times of stress (Kems, Klepac, & Cole, 1996; Larson, Richards, Moneta, Holmbeck, & Duckett, 1996). From an adolescent developmental perspective, researchers found that adolescent development tends to proceed best when adolescents develop some autonomy from parents, while simultaneously maintaining an ongoing supportive and close parent- adolescent relationship (Grotevant, 1989; Grotevant & Cooper, 1986). The experience of entering college has been described as a naturally occurring separation experience that activates attachment-related processing because the adolescent must navigate, explore, and master a novel environment (Kenny, 1987). A number of studies have shown that students securely attached to their parents report better social, academic and emotional well-being in college (Bradford & Lyddon, 1993; Holmbreck & Wandrei, 1993; Kenny & Perez, 1996; Larose & Boivin, 1998; Rice, et al, 1995; Schultheiss & Blustein, 1994), greater social connectedness with friends and less loneliness (Blain, Thompson & Whiffen, 1993; Brack, Gay, & Matheny, 1993), and greater psychological well- being (Arrnsden 8: Greenberg, 1987; Cavell, Jones, Runyan, Constantin-Page, & Velasquez, 1993; Vrvona, 2000). The symbolic or physical presence of parental attachment figures can create conditions of safety, security, and guidance that enable students to explore the novel environment with feelings of confidence (Holmbreck & Wandrei, 1993; Papini & Roggman, 1992) and obtain mastery so that students are less likely to respond to challenges with disappointment and 11 feelings of helplessness (Annsden & Greenberg, 1987). The degree to which students exhibit well-being is strongly related to their style of parental attachment (Klasner 8. Pistole, 2003). Although studies have included students from a variety of ethnic backgrounds, the influence of attachment theory on the well—being of college students has been studied mostly among white middle-class college students (Holmbreck & Wandrei, 1993; Kenny, 1987; Kenny & Donaldson, 1991; Rice 8. Cummins, 1996). As a result, little is known about attachment and well-being among nonmajority racial/ethnic groups (Kenny 8. Rice, 1995). For African American students at predominantly White universities, some researchers (Dewitt-Parker, 2000; Duesterhaus, 2004; Hinderiie & Kenny, 2002) found that parental attachment type was predictive of college well-being, the development of life goals, and the development of extrinsic aspirations. Other researchers have found secure parental attachment positively associated with psychological well-being among racially and ethnically diverse college students, including Hispanic and African American students (Kenny & Perez, 1996; Taub, 1995). These findings are consistent with the relationships found between parental attachment style and late-adolescent well-being among Caucasian samples. Additional evaluation of the influence of parental attachment style on late- adolescent well-being is warranted, particularly among majority and nonmajority students from divorced families. In this study, attachment to parents was examined by comparing mother-attachment and father-attachment with regard to 12 three subscales: feelings of affects, parents’ promotions of independence, and parents as sources of support. Other Background Variables In addition to age, several other factors are frequently examined and/or controlled for in studies involving parental attachment and divorce. Potential covariates noted in the literature included postdivorce marital status (Lopez et al., 2000), frequency of father contact (Hetherington, et al., 1982; Wallerstein & Kelly, 1980), socio—economic status factors (Whiteside & Becker, 2000), current living arrangements (McCormick & Kennedy, 2000), time since divorce (Lewis, Feiring, & Rosenthal, 2000), number of children in household, household income, parental educations, and age (Hanson, 1999). All participants provided the following information: age, gender, race, socio-economic status (SES), educational level, SES and educational level of both parents, age at time of parent's separation, age at time of parent’s divorce, length of the marriage, years resided in one-adult home, current living situation (e.g., parent, self, other), number of siblings, birth order, parents current marital status, age at time of remarriage, frequency of father contact currently and at time of divorce, and a description of contact with father. In addition, as measures of well-being may be confounded by social desirability (i.e., tendencies to answer questions in terms of social appropriateness rather than true well—being) and ethnic identity (i.e., attitudes and knowledge of who one is as a member of an ethnic group), a social desirability scale and ethnic identity measure was included in the data collection. After data was collected, it was analyzed to determine covariates. Covariates, 13 namely, unwanted variance, were controlled in the final analysis and are discussed in the results section. Parental Attachment and Adolescent Well-being The concept of well-being has been defined and measured in many different ways, by many different researchers (Adams, Bezner, & Drabbs, 2000; Adams, Bezner, & Steinhardt, 1997; Ardell, 1986; Depken, 1994; Greenburg, 1985; Travis 8. Callander, 1990). Within adolescent research, strong emphasis is placed on psychological and spiritual dimensions of well-being. Therefore, the dimensions of psychologiml and spiritual well—being will be measured in this study, in addition to the physical, social, emotional, and intellectual dimensions of well-being. Noticeable emphasis on spirituality has been evident in the literature and research over the past fifty years (refer to Adams et al., 2000 for a review). Dunn stated in 1966 that “we can no longer ignore the spirit... as a factor in our medical and health disciplines... which are designed as though the sum total of our concern is for the body and the mind... leaving to metaphysics and religion the affairs of the spirit" (p. 216). Over the past couple decades, several researchers have included spirituality as a dimension of well-being (Chapman, 1987; Depken, 1994; Myers, 1992; Travis & Callander, 1990; Whitmer 8. Sweeney, 1992). Although the conceptualizations of the spiritual dimension vary among researchers, 3 common thread includes a sense of meaning and purpose in life (Adams et al., 2000; Chapman, 1987; Hettler, 1984; Seaward, 1995; Whitmer & Sweeney, 1992). Krippner (2005) defined spiritual as “an awareness 14 of a broader life meaning that transcends the immediacy of everyday physical expediency” (p. 225). Likewise, Paulson (2005) defined spirituality as an awareness of a broader life meaning, extending beyond everyday matters and natural concerns. The conceptualization of spiritual well-being in this manner allowed students to respond to the spiritual questionnaire from either a secular or religious context. Based on their own belief systems, the students decided individually whether their sense of meaning and purpose in life was fulfilled via mystical peak experiences, connection to other people, linkage to a greater power than the self, or some other source. In this study, the spiritual dimension of well-being was defined as a positive sense of meaning and purpose in life. Self-esteem has been empirically shown to be a good indicator of general psychological health and will be used in this study to measure adolescents’ psychological well-being. In studies of adolescent’s well-being, the adolescents self-esteem is frequently assessed (Kenny, Lomax, Brabeck, 8: Fife, 1998; Leonardi 8. Kiosseoglou, 2000; McCormick, & Kennedy, 2000; Meyers, 1998). Self-esteem refers to an attitude of self-acceptance, self-approval, self-respect, and self-worth. Whereas parental divorce has been shown to have long-term detrimental effects on self-esteem (Billingham & Abrahams, 1998), several studies have shown that the level of adolescents’ self-esteem is directly related to the style of attachment to parents (Carranza & Kilmann, 2000; Hojat, 1998; McCormick, & Kennedy, 2000; Meyers, 1998). Carranza and Kilmann (2000) found that young women in intact families who perceive their fathers as unapproachable and unsupportive demonstrate insecure attachment patterns 15 arc 3:3 wl‘.’ pate sci), and lower levels of self-esteem than those that perceive their fathers as approachable and supportive. In addition, Hojat (1998) reported that satisfaction with mother was significantly associated with higher self-esteem in their children. McCurdy and Scherrnan (1996) found that adolescents who have experienmd parental divorce and subsequent mother-custody arrangements are at risk for high conflict and poor quality of emotional attachment with fathers, which could limit their overall sense of well—being. Thus, the assertion that a positive relationship with mother is a protective factor against the adverse effects of divorce (McCombs 8 Forehand, 1989) has been contradicted by the finding that a conflictual relationship with even one parent is associated with poorer well- being (Lapsley, Rice, & Shadid, 1989; McCurdy & Scherrnan, 1996). Adolescent well-being appears to be associated with attachment to fathers, especially in the subscales of the affective quality of the relationship and fathers as sources of support (McCurdy & Scherrnan, 1996). The relationship between adolescents’ well-being and style of attachment with custodial mothers and with noncustodial fathers has supplied valuable information regarding the adolescents’ overall adjustment to the divorce environment and has provided insight and direction for practitioners working with families, divorcing parents, and adolescents. Research Questions Numerous attempts have been made to account for the differences in children’s reactions to parental divorce. Variables such as maternal stress, parenting style, conflict between parents, absent parents, custody arrangements, schedules, economic resources, parental behavior, attachment quality and 16 environmental factors (e.g., remarriage, father adjustment, mother adjustment) have all been researched (Whiteside & Becker, 2000). However, few, if any, studies have simultaneously examined parent—adolescent attachment style, maternal conflict with father, gender, well-being and the interactional effects between these variables among African American, Hispanic, and Caucasian adolescents. The goal in this study was first to examine the main effects and interactive effects of style of maternal attachment and maternal conflict with father on the style of paternal attachment within each racial/ethnic group. This shed light on whether postdivorce style of matemal—adolescent attachment and maternal conflict with father each contribute important and unique variance to predictions of the style of the father-adolescent attachment and whether the interaction of these factors also contributes significant variance to these predictors. Secondly, an evaluation of the adolescents’ general, psychological, and spiritual well-being provided some indication of the adolescents’ internalization of the main effects and suggested possible impacts of these effects. Thirdly, other background variables (e.g., social desirability and ethnic identity) that were significantly related to focal variables were controlled for in order to enhance the overall predictive power of the results. Given these objectives, the following questions were addressed within and between each of the racial/ethnic groups: 1. Among adolescents from divorced families, is there a relationship between a) the style of maternal attachment and the style of paternal attachment, b) the degree of maternal conflict with father and the style of paternal l7 F.) 3 Sun: "We: la'nl atar afid attachment, c) the style of maternal attachment and adolescents well- being, and d) the style of paternal attachment and adolescents well-being? 2. Among adolescents from divorced families, controlling for the quality of maternal attachment, does the degree of maternal conflict with father uniquely predict the style of paternal attachment? 3. Does the style of maternal attachment moderate the relationship between the degree of maternal conflict with father and the style of paternal attachment? 4. Among adolescents from divorced families, holding background variables constant, does maternal attachment style and maternal conflict with father uniquely predict the relationship between the style of paternal attachment and adolescent well-being? Summary and Hypotheses Attachment theory provides a strong conceptual framework from which to investigate the relational bond between parents and adolescents from divorced families. Yet, little research has been done applying the basic assumptions of attachment theory to the explanation of why some adolescents experience long- term effects of divorce while other adolescents do not. Even less research has been done in this area with postdivorce African American and Hispanic youth. In this study, the impact custodial mothers have on father-adolescent relationships and adolescent well-being was evaluated for African American, Hispanic and Caucasian adolescents. 18 and ate . The purpose of this study was to explore the associations of both 1) maternal attachment and 2) maternal conflict with father with paternal attachment and well-being among adolescents from divorced families. Drawing from attachment theory and the literature on divorce, it was hypothesized that: 1. 3. Among adolescents from divorced families, secure attachment to mother and low levels of maternal conflict with father would correspond with secure attachment to father. Conversely, insecure attachment to mother and high levels of maternal conflict with father would correspond with insecure attachment to father. . Secure attachments to parents would be associated with higher levels of general, spiritual, and psychological well-being, while insecure attachments to parents would be associated with lower levels of general, spiritual, and psychological well-being. In regards to race/ethnicity, it was hypothesized that: a. No significant differences would be found differentiating Hispanic and African American adolescents’ from Caucasian adolescents’ parental attachment patterns. b. Hispanic and African American adolescents’ would demonstrate significantly more adaptive coping strategies to insecure parental attachment style and maternal conflict with father as evidenced by greater overall well-being than the Caucasian adolescents’ with insecure parental attachment style and higher levels of conflict. 19 Literature Review While the majority of adolescents from divorced families fall into the normal range on measures of both psychological and cognitive functioning (Kelly, 2000), many adolescents show prolonged negative reactions to the divorce process, and great variability exists amongst both of these groups (Jekielek, 1998; Seltzer, 1994; Whiteside & Becker, 2000). Parental divorce during childhood may adversely impact the style of the parent-child relationship, leading to long-term psychosocial adjustment difficulties (Emery & Forehand, 1994). The literature indicates that parental divorce presents unique obstacles to the development and maintenance of positive parental relationships, especially with noncustodial fathers. This section will review six lines of research relevant to the proposed study: 1) postdivorce father involvement and father-child relationships, 2) mother-child, father-child, and mother-father relationships, 3) parental conflict and child well-being, 4) attachment theory, 5) age and attachment, and 6) race/ethnicity. Postdivorce Father Involvement and Father-Child Relationships The population of divorced Americans continues to be substantially high, estimated at nearly 20 million in 1998, up from 11 million in 1980 (Raymond, 2001). Since 1960, the divorce rate in the United States has more than doubled and the last-reported U.S. divorce rate as of May, 2005, is 38% (National Center for Health Statistics, 2005). A meta-analysis of 37 studies examining the long- terrn relationship of parental divorce on children and adolescents found they were more likely to have psychological and social difficulties and to score lower 20 on measures of the quality of mother-child and father-child relationships (Amato & Keith, 1991). One proposed explanatory idea for these findings posits that the negative relationship between parental divorce and the adjustment of adolescents results from disrupted parent-child relationships (Emery & Forehand, 1994). Although the potentially negative effects of divorce can be mitigated by children maintaining good relationships with both parents (Barnes, 1999), several studies indimte a precipitous decline in father contact after divorce, with 23% to 30% of sampled children and sampled fathers reporting no contact with each other during the first year postdivorce (Furstenberg, Morgan, 8. Allison, 1987; Mitchell, 1985; Seltzer, Schaeffer, & Chamg, 1989). Attempts to elucidate the reasons for the decrease in noncustodial father-child contact after divorce have been minimal (Dudley, 1991), with most efforts identifying loosely connected predictors of father involvement (Stone & McKenry, 1998). However, since fathers with high frequency of visitation are more likely to have high-quality relationships with their children (Whiteside & Becker, 2000), and high-quality relationships are positively related to children’s well-being (Hetherington, Cox, & Cox, 1982), lack of father involvement following divorce needs to be understood. To do this, the father-child relationship needs to be examined in the context of both the level of father’s involvement and the relationship between the parents. The research conducted by Hetherington, Cox, and Cox (1982) and by Wallerstein and Kelly (1980) concluded that in the two years following parental separation, positive adjustment for children is associated with supportive coparenting and low conflict between parents, whereas child difficulties are 21 associated with disagreement and inconsistency between parents and continued anger between parents. While supportive coparenting and low conflict between postdivorce parents is ideal, this request may present a tall order for parents to fill. No matter how smoothly parents manage the divorce process, when they part, they face the reality that the time children spend with one parent inevitably mandates separation from the other parent. The impact of this appears to be particularly relevant in regard to maternal role identity. Families with high paternal involvement and cooperation correspondingly have less residential time with mother and mothers may oppose the impact this has on their relationship with their children. For example, Little (1992) found that mothers were more satisfied with parenting when they had primary custody than when they have joint custody, indicating a preference for more maternal control of the mother-child and the father-child relationships. Kruk (1993) identified ways in which the custodial mother discouraged father contact and Lehr and MacMillan (2001) found that poor father relationships with custodial mother resulted in differences in access to the children as well as loss of day-to-day contact with them. These findings highlight the importance of evaluating the father-child relationship in relation to the mother-father dyad and the mother-child relationship. Mother-Child, Father-Child, and Mother-Father Relationships While most studies have focused on either the parent-child or the mother- father relationship, theorists and investigators have increasingly recognized that the different dyads within the family (mother—child, father-child, mother-father) are mutually interdependent (Cook, 1994; Lytton, 1990). A meta-analysis of 11 22 35 c? EX TE; let ha: tor: L." We studies involving 672 participants found that attachment to mother and attachment to father were significantly related (Fox, Kimmerly, & Schafer, 1991 ). In another meta-analysis of 12 studies, researchers report consistent positive correlations between mother-child attachment style and father-child attachment style (Whiteside & Becker, 2000). This means that when one parent has a good relationship with the child, it is likely that the other parent does also. Conversely, children classified as secure to one parent are highly unlikely to be classified as insecure to the other parent. Interdependence is found between the mother-child relationships, the father-child relationships, and the mother-father relationship’s impact on child behaviors, yet each has unique distinctive influences. For example, Mathijssen, Koot, Berhulst, DeBruyn, and Oud assessed the relative association of the different family dyads and discovered that both the mother- child and the mother-father relationship were positively related to child behavior. However, whereas the mother-child relationship was consistently related to externalizing behavior of children, the mother-father relationship was particularly related to internalizing behavior of children. Offer, Ostrov, & Howard (1998) found that both mother-child and father-child relationship style significantly correlated with adolescents’ self-image; however, the mother-child relationship had a greater influence. Miller and Stubblefleld (1993) concluded that disclosure to mothers was more common than disclosure to fathers, but the strongest predictor of disclosure to one parent was disclosure to the other parent. These studies illustrate that the influence of family relations on the child cannot fully be understood by studying one dyad in isolation. Whiteside (1998) found that 23 althoug Wow: the co; reéatc: dyfiam Comm Parent m Pores? “WI 330295 bene- Cox. 8 and A: £2" :3 “-u 1 although a substantial number of studies examined the mother-child dyad following divorce, fewer have included information about the father-child dyad or the coparent alliance. The parent-child relationships and mother-father relationship must each be included when examining the relationships of family dynamics and between family dynamics and child well-being (Davies 8. Cummings, 1994; Rothbaum & Weisz, 1994). Parental Conflict Interparental conflict and the parent-child relationship have each been associated with child adjustment after parental divorce (Amato, 1993; Emery 8. Forehand, 1994; Summers et al., 1998). Supportive co-parenting, low conflict between parents, and better personal adjustment of parents are associated with adolescent psychosocial adjustment, while continued anger and disagreement between parents have been associated with adolescent difficulties (Hetherington, Cox, 8. Cox, 1982; Wallerstein & Kelly, 1980). According to Kurkowski, Gordon, and Arbuthnot (1993), children from divorced families are caught in the middle of parental conflicts significantly more often and experience more stress than children from intact homes. Ahrons (1994) found that fifty percent of white, middle—class divorced couples engage in bitter, open conflict and only twelve percent were able to create pleasant, low-conflict relationships after the divorce. Interparental conflict has been identified consistently as a major correlate of behavior problems in children across a wide array of family structures and settings (for reviews Davies & Cummings, 1994; Erel & Burman, 1995). In a recent meta-analysis, Amato and Keith (1991) found that parental conflict 24 dive! (He: Cont mfoh accounted for more of the negative consequences of divorce than both parental absence and economic disadvantage. Interparental conflict plays a central role in the frequency of father visitation and consequently with the father-child relationship style (Whiteside & Becker, 2000). A meta-analysis of 57 studies focusing on the style of paternal involvement indicated that enduring emotional bonds are more strongly associated with positive child outcomes than with frequency of paternal contact (Amato & Gilbreth, 1999). However, without spending time with their children, it is less likely that fathers will have the opportunity to develop and/or maintain a quality relationship with their children. The level of father involvement after divorce can be substantially affected by the co—parental relationship (Hetherington, Cox, & Cox, 1982; McKenry, Price, Fine, & Servovich, 1992). Conflict with the former spouse is one of the major barriers to maintaining father involvement (lhinger-Tallman, Pasley, & Buehler, 1993). Continuing conflict with the ex-wife has been shown to impact both how fathers feel about their children and what they do in relation to them (Goldberg, 1990). Therefore, it is not surprising that parental conflict is related to insecure attachments (Lewis, Fairing, & Rosenthal, 2000), but it is interesting that conflictual mother-father relations have more negative effects on father-child relations than on mother-child relations (Cummings 8: Watson-O’Reilly, 1997). Similarly, a meta-analysis conducted by Whiteside and Becker (2000) demonstrated that the impact of cooperation between parents and the level of faflwer’s involvement with children were different for the mother-child relationship 25 than for the father-child relationship. Cooperative and supportive coparenting skills were significantly associated with greater frequency of father visits and father-child relationship style, whereas cooperation and greater frequency of father visits negatively related to the mother-child relationship style (Whiteside & Becker, 2000). Fathers that were less collaborative were associated with mothers that exhibited more warmth and support of children (Whiteside & Becker, 2000). Orbuch, Thornton, and Cancio (2000) found that the mother- daughter relationship improved after divorce. Therefore, even though high conflict between parents interferes with the mother-child and father-child relationships, cooperative parenting and high paternal involvement may correspond with mothers having less time with their children and feeling less validated in their role as a mother. Thus, a more complex relationship between divorce, parental conflict, and child well-being is implied. If mothers benefit from less father involvement and poor father-child relationships, mothers may be consciously or subconsciously sabotaging their children’s relationship with their fathers. Hanson’s (1999) results suggested that children’s exposure to conflict accounted for about 11% of the effects of divorce on child well-being, suggesting that conflict only partially explains why parental divorce and child well-being are related. In this study, the construct of maternal conflict with father was measured by examining adolescent perceptions of maternal hostility toward father, maternal facilitation! interference of paternal involvement, and cooperative parenting behaviors of mothers. Attachment Theory 26 Over the last several decades, attachment theory (Bowlby, 1969) has served as an important framework for conceptualizing the parent-child relationship. Attachment theory is based on the notion that there are individual differences in the way infants become emotionally attached to their primary caregivers and that these differences influence a child’s perceptions of self, others, and resources for emotional self-regulation in times of crisis (Bowlby, 1969). An infant’s relationship with its caretakers evolves into a working model that organizes behavior (Ainsworth & Bowlby, 1991). Initially, attachment behavior manifests in crying, smiling, and vocalizing which serve to keep the infant in close proximity to the caregiver for purposes of protection (Bowlby, 1969). During the early stages of development, the infant forms internal representations of self, attachment figure, and the environment (Bowlby, 1969). The attachment system thereby becomes a component of the cognitive structure subject to developmental change as well as environmental influence (Ainsworth, 1 989). One basic principle of attachment theory is that attachment relationships continue to be important throughout the lifespan (Ainsworth, 1989). If the caregiver is experienced as warm, responsive, dependable, and encouraging of autonomy, secure attachment characterizes the relationship (Hazan & Shaver, 1987). However, if the caregiver is experienced as cold, neglectful, inconsistent, rejecting, or intrusive, an insecure attachment is likely to characterize the relationship (Hazan & Shaver, 1987). Assessments of attachment relations during adolescence have indiwted continuity of both mother- and father-child 27 attachment over time (Lopez, 1996; McCormick & Kennedy, 1994), as well as similar proportions of persons in each attachment category as reported in infancy (Hazan & Shaver, 1987). Attachment security has been shown repeatedly to be an important correlate of social and emotional adjustment throughout the life span (Bartholomew & Horowitz, 1991). One way to look at the caregiving environment is to examine the effects of separation/divorce on attachment styles. It is widely recognized that the caregiving environment, particularly maternal behavior and characteristics, is related to continuity of the attachment classification (Egeland & Farber, 1984). However, parental separation/divorce captures many aspects of the caregiving environment because it has a direct impact on parents, children, and the emotional and social experiences in the family (Davies, & Cummings, 1994). The decision to separate sets in motion a significant and potentially stressful transition that frequently includes intense negative emotion and disruption of everyday structures and routines. Thus separation/divorce provides the type of environment that impacts early childhood attachment as well as effecting change in attachment representation (Lewis, et al., 2000). Subsequently, we know that the processes associated with the marital separation experience tend to change the parent-child relationship, but it is difficult to ascertain exactly which variables are significant in this transformation of parent-child relationships. So the parental behaviors and the coparenting partnership should be considered jointly to understand the full set of influences on the child. 28 Points in time of parental separation and of parental divorce are seldom differentiated within the divorce literature and the terms are used interchangably. Several of the more recent studies use both terms in reference to the breakup of a parental relationship. This may be in response to the growing number of parents that do not go through the legal process of marriage, and as a result, do not go through the legal process of a divorce. A child in a single-parent home in 1996 was as likely to be living with a parent who had never been married (36%) as with a parent who was divorced (37%) (US. Census Bureau, 2001). In the current study, participants’ age at time of separation and age at time of divorce was requested and will be evaluated. Age and Attachment In an attempt to identify predictors of college adjustment, researchers have studied the influence of secure parental attachment (An'nsden & Greenberg, 1987; Brack, Gay, & Matheny, 1993; Bradford & Lyddon, 1993; Kenny, 1987; Kenny 8. Donaldson, 1991; Larose and Boivin, 1998; Mattanah, et al., 2004; Rice, et al., 1995; Vivona, 2000). A number of psychologists have emphasized the importance of autonomy and individuation as key developmental tasks facing late-adolescent college-students (Arnstein, 1980; Chickering, 1969). It has been proposed that students with a stronger and healthier sense of self will be better equipped to handle the challenge of disengaging from childhood and learning to function in the college environment as autonomous adults, including self-organization of coursework and preparation for exams, negotiating new social environments and relationships, and attention to career and identity issues 29 (Grayson, 1998; Kenny & Rice, 1995). From an adolescent developmental perspective, researchers found that adolescent development tends to proceed best when adolescents develop some autonomy from parents, while simultaneously maintaining an ongoing supportive and close parent-adolescent relationship (Grotevant, 1989; Grotevant & Cooper, 1985). Conversely, those adolescents who strive too strongly to separate from their parents appear isolated and withdrawn and are at increased risk of behavioral problems (Ryan & Lynch, 1989). Attachment is defined as an enduring emotional bond that forms between the parent and the child across the life span (Rice, et al., 1995) and secure parental attachment helps the adolescent develop a positive view of self and expect positive, supportive interactions with others (Griffin & Bartholomew, 1 994). The experience of entering college has been described as a naturally occurring separation experience that activates attachment-related processing because the adolescent must navigate, explore, and master a novel environment (Kenny, 1987). The college transition may be viewed correspondingly as a second “strange situation” (Kenny, 1990), in which adolescents with a history of secure attachment to their parents experience them as a secure base that anchors and supports exploratory behavior and provides anxiety-reducing functions (Baldwin & Fehr, 1995; Elicker, Englund, Sroufe, 1992; Hazan & Shaver, 1994). A number of studies have shown that students securely attached to their parents report better social, academic and emotional adjustment in college (Bradford & Lyddon, 1993; Holmbreck & Wandrei, 1993; Kenny & Perez, 3O 1996; Larose 8 Boivin, 1998; Rice, et al, 1995; Schultheiss 8 Blustein, 1994), greater social connectedness with friends and less loneliness (Blain, Thompson 8 Whiffen, 1993; Brack, Gay, 8 Matheny, 1993), less psychologically distressing symptoms such as depression and anxiety, and less alcohol use (Arrnsden 8 Greenberg, 1987; Cavell, Jones, Runyan, Constantin-Page, 8 Velasquez, 1993; Vivona, 2000). The symbolic or physical presence of parental attachment figures can create conditions of safety, security, and guidance that enable students to explore the novel environment with feelings of confidence (Holmbreck 8 Wandrei, 1993; Papini 8 Roggman, 1992). Attachment security provides the resource for mastery so that students are less likely to respond to challenges with disappointment and feelings of helplessness (Arrnsden 8 Greenberg, 1987). In addition, students having the expectation that others will be helpful and responsive are more likely to approach faculty for assistance and to develop supportive friendships (Florian, Mikulincer, 8 Bucholtz, 1995; Lopez, 1996). Thus the degree to which students’ well-being is resilient to the college transition is strongly related to their style of parental attachment (Klasner 8 Pistole, 2003). Although studies have included students from a variety of ethnic backgrounds, the influence of attachment theory on the well-being of college students has been studied mostly among white middle-class college students (Holmbreck 8 Wandrei, 1993; Kenny, 1987; Kenny 8 Donaldson, 1991; Rice 8 Cummins, 1996). As a result, little is know about attachment and wellobeing among nonmajority racial/ethnic groups (Kenny 8 Rice, 1995). In a study of attachment and psychological well—being among racially and ethnically diverse 31 first-year college students, Kenny and Perez (1996) found the secure parental attachment was negatively associated with psychological symptoms of distress. For African American students at predominantly White universities, other researchers (Dewitt-Parker, 2000; Duesterhaus, 2004; Hinderlie 8 Kenny, 2002) found that parental attachment type was predictive of college adjustment, the development of life goals, and the development of extrinsic aspirations. Taub (1995) found that among Hispanic, African American and Caucasian students, one of the most significant independent contributors to autonomy among college students was the parental role of providing emotional support. These findings are consistent with the associations found between parental attachment style and late-adolescent well-being among white samples. Additional evaluation of the influence of parental attachment style on late- adolescent well-being is warranted, particularly among majority and nonmajority students from divorced families. College students from intact families may perceive their parents as a greater source of support, display more career decidedness, and exhibit more financial connectedness than students whose parents are divorced (Scott, 1999). However, studies have found that growing up with secure parent attachment, as compared to a preoccupied or dismissing attachment style, may increase an adolescents resilience to possible negative effects of parental divorce (Hannum 8 Dvorak, 2004; Hayashi 8 Strickland, 1998). In this study, attachment to parents was examined by comparing mother- attachment and father-attachment with regard to three subscales: feelings of affects, parental promotion of independence, and parents as sources of support. 32 Race/Ethnicity and Attachment As previously mentioned, the majority of research on divorce has focused on homogeneous samples drawn from White, middle-class contexts, with little attention given to African American or Hispanic marriages (Feldman 8 Elliott, 1990; Guidubaldi, 1988; Hines, 1997; Ogbu, 1988; Power, Hauser, 8 Kilner, 1989). According to Billingsley (1990), relatively little attention has been given to the impact of divorce on African Americans because of researchers’ inability to dispel the pathological approach to black families which emphasizes single- parent families and teen pregnancies. Although African Americans are associated with unique types of family patterns, most studies on Black families have de-emphasized internal family dynamics and have focused on family pathologies (Billingsley, 1990, Crawley 1988). Statistics indicate that approximately 47% of African American women separate from their husbands within 10-15 years of marriage, compared with 28% of Caucasian women. Some researchers use this phenomenon to guide their research and explain their results. For example, Shaw, Winslow, and Flanagan (1999) stated, “We also expected the effects of pre- and postdivorce marital status to be more pronounced among European American rather than African American children, due to the greater prevalence of two-parent families among European American families. That is, single—parent status may be accepted as more normative among 33 African American children regardless of whether or not a divorce has occurred” (p. 744). The approach to African American families as a deviant form has obscured the diversity of African American families based on socialization experiences, geographical regions, and socioeconomic backgrounds (Lawson 8 Thomson, 1994). According to Ainsworth (1989), the attachment model is applicable across racial/ethnic settings, but some researchers have questioned the ethnic specificity and measure of the attachment construct (Le Vine 8 Miller, 1990). There is some evidence that African American adolescents may be particularly sensitive to interpersonal matters and family relationships (DeCindio, Floyd, Wilcox, 8 McSevency, 1983; Hughes 8 Demo, 1989) and that they perceive higher levels of intimacy with parents than do Caucasian adolescents (Cemkovich 8 Giordano, 1987), even after controlling for socioeconomic status (Giordano, Cemkovich, 8 DeMaris, 1993). African American adolescents have been found to value relationships with parents, particularly mothers, more highly than Caucasian adolescents (D’Augelli 8 Hershberger, 1992). Figures from the US. Bureau of the Census (2000) indicate that African American and Hispanic children will spend significantly longer periods in a single-parent, mother-headed household than Caucasian children. In contrast to the above mentioned results, a meta-analysis of the long- term consequences of parental divorce for adolescent well-being found that the effect sizes tended to be stronger for Caucasians than for African Americans for separation and divorce, one-parent family status, material quality of life, 34 occupational quality, and educational attainment (Amato 8 Keith, 1991). Caucasian samples were associated with significantly stronger negative effect sizes than were African American or mixed-race samples (b = .14 and -.43, respectively; Amato 8 Keith, 1991). Other studies have found racial/ethnic similarities on measures of adolescent-parent relationships (Rice, Cunningham, 8 Young, 1997; Veneziano 8 Rohner, 1998; Zimmerman, Salem, 8 Maton, 1995). For example, in a study of 21 African American and 37 Caucasian fathers and their children, Veneziano and Rohner (1998) found that only perceived paternal acceptance was significantly related to African American and Caucasian children’s psychological adjustment and that father involvement by itself was not related to either group’s psychological adjustment. Regarding marital conflict, African American women seem to exhibit greater levels of disrupted parenting before divorce than afterward, whereas Caucasian mothers tended to show more disrupted parenting following the marital separation than before (Shaw, Winslow, 8 Flanagan, 1999). These findings may be significant in explaining why African American females experienced less depression and adjusted more positively to single-parenthood than their Caucasian counterparts (Gove 8 Shin, 1989; Menaghan 8 Lieberman, 1986; Weiss, 1975). Lawson and Thomson (1994) found that African American men reported postmarital friendship relationships with former spouses and that these relationships satisfied their need for attachment, perhaps through shared parenting and social networks. This relationship between African Americans postdivorce mother-father dyads warrants further investigation. 35 Whereas the research on postdivorce parental relationships and adolescent well-being is limited for African Americans, it is virtually absent for Hispanic adolescents. In one study, Lindahl and Malik (1999) found more similarities than differences between Hispanic American and Caucasian families in a study of intact families, reporting that marital conflict and disengaged family alliances were associated with child behavior problems and dysfunctional patterns of family interaction in both ethnic groups. They concluded, “although some of the patterns of interrelatedness are influenced by culture, many are not, suggesting that ethnic differences may somehow, but not always, affect how family subsystems are related to one another” (Lindahl 8 Malik, 1999; p. 22). One significant difference Lindahl and Malik (1999) found was that a hierarchical parenting style was associated with boys’ behavioral problems for Caucasian families, but not for Hispanic families. In Hispanic families, only lax and inconsistent parenting was associated with externalizing behaviors. This finding is supported by a study done by Zayas and Solari (1994). In exploring the development of adaptive strategies, Zayas and Solari (1994) found Hispanic families place a lot of emphasis on family solidarity and on individuals’ sense of obligation to the family. A parenting practice that incorporates these goals involves the insistence on children’s conformity to parental and extended family authority (Zayas 8 Palleja, 1988). This strategy promotes the importance of family relatedness and helps in racial identity development of Hispanic children. Parental divorce may disrupt this adaptive strategy that helps to not only protect the family’s continuity, but also to 36 preserves its culture. A goal of this study was to gain insight into internal family dynamics and extend the attachment literature to Hispanic families. Healthy well-being, which is influenced by parental attachment style, is culturally defined (Rothman, Weisz, Pott, Mijake, 8 Morelli, 2000) and should be evaluated within cultural contexts. Parenting practices are embedded within the cultural context of the parent-child relationship and greater attachment-related autonomy or proximity is encouraged as an element of the working model and attachment style of that culture (Van lJzendoorn 8 Sagi, 1999). For example, the pattern of establishing extended families appears to be an adaptive strategy common to African American and Hispanic individuals (Harrison, Wilson, Pine, Chan, 8 Buriel, 1990). Three times as many African American children under the age of 18 live with their grandparents as do Caucasian Americans (US. Bureau of the Census, 1996). Kenny and Perez (1996) found that 27% of minority students identified their primary attachment figure as someone other than a parent. A caregiving environment that consists of a network of more or less stable relationships between the individual and several caregivers may be optimal (Donley, 1993) and may shelter the adolescent from some of the negative consequences associated with parental divorce, including insecure attachment style and maternal conflict with father. An understanding of the network of stable relationships may be important to obtaining a culturally infused theory of attachment. In this study, racial/ethnic differences in postdivorce parental relationships and well-being among African American, Hispanic, and Caucasian adolescents 37 were explored. Though some inconsistencies exist, a synthesis of the literature appears to support the postulate that no significant differences will be found between Hispanic and African American adolescents’ and Caucasian adolescents’ pattern of attachment. According to Bakermans-Kranenburg, Van lJzendoorn, and Kroonenberg (2004), children of African American, Hispanic, and Caucasian families in the United States may be exposed to culturally specific experiences, but these do not alter the relationship between attachment security and emotional responsiveness and support of parents. However the research also appears to support the postulate that college students of color may be more resilient and less affected by the stressors associated with parental divorce. Summary Attachment theory should provide a strong conceptual framework for investigating the relational bond between parents and adolescents from divorced families. The purpose of this study is to explain why some adolescents fare well after the divorce of their parents while others do not. One underlying assumption of this study is that postdivorce relationships with both parents are important for the well—being of children. Parental divorce may adversely impact the quality of the parent-child relationship, leading to long-term psychosocial adjustment difficulties (Emery 8 Forehand, 1994). The literature indicates that parental divorce presents unique obstacles to the development and maintenance of positive parental relationships and overall well-being. Few studies have evaluated the impact of parental divorce and maternal conflict with fathers on parental attachment and the subsequent influence on adolescents’ well-being. 38 The cunent study helps to fill this gap in the literature. Additionally, this study extends the attachment literature relevant to racial/ethnic groups and clarifies some of the inconsistencies reported in previous research. 39 Method Participants The participants were 179 (141 female and 38 male) students recruited from a major research institution located in an urban setting (population exceeding 250,000) in the Midwest. The racial/ethnic composition was 52% White (n = 93; 76 female, 17 male), 32.4% African American (n = 58; 47 female, 11 male), and 15.6% Hispanic/LatinolMexican American (n = 28; 18 female, 10 male). Age ranged from 18—24 (mean = 19.97). Education level Included 66 freshman (36.8%), 41 sophomores (22.9%), 42 juniors (23.5%), 27 seniors (15.0%), and 3 graduate students (1.7%). All participants were from families in which biological parents divorced prior to age 16 and mothers had primary physical custody. Age of participant at time of parents’ separation ranged from age one to age 16 (n = 122, mean = 7.3) and age of participants at time of parents’ divorce ranged from birth to age 16 (n = 179, mean = 7.2 years). Fifty- seven students failed to indicate their age at the time of their parents divorce, but for those that did, no significant difference was found between age at separation and age at time of divorce (Cohen’s d = .016; effect size = .008). Frequency of contact with father prior to age 18 included daily (n = 39, 22%), weekly (n = 73, 41%), monthly (n = 37, 21%), and a couple times a year (n = 30, 17%). Student participants were also single with no children and had at least an 8th grade reading level. Measures All participants completed the following self-report measures. 40 Personal data questionnaire (PDQ). This questionnaire solicited demographic information from participants as well as information related to parents’ divorce. The questionnaire included information on adolescents’ age, gender, race, educational level, educational level of both parents, age at time of parents’ separation, age at time of parents” divorce, length of the marriage, years resided in one-adult home, number of siblings, parents’ current marital status, age at time of remarriage, frequency of contact with mother and with father currently and at time of divorce, and a brief description of contact with father. Parental Attachment Questionnaire (PA Q; Kenny, 1 98 7). Parental attachment was operationalized along a continuous scale indicating the degree of maternal attachment and paternal attachment with regard to three subscales: feelings of affect, parental promotion of independence, and parents as sources of support. This measure provided full- scale attachment scores and individual subscale scores. The Parental Attachment Questionnaire is a 55-item Likert scale designed to assess the perceptions of young adults about their parents’ attitudes towards them and their relationships with their parents (Kenny, 1987). F ifty-two of the fifty-five items were used for this study. The three items that were omitted did not specifically address maternal and paternal attachment (e.g., “When I have a serious problem or an important decision to make, I work it out on my own without help,” “When I have a serious problem or an important decision to make, I discuss the matter with a friend,” and “When I have a serious problem or an 41 important decision to make, I talk to a professional, such as a psychologist, member of the clergy, etc”). The PAQ contains three subscales: affective quality of attachment, parental fostering of autonomy, and parental role in providing emotional support. Subjects answer each question using a five-point rating scale ranging from 1 (not at all) to 5 (very much). Scores are calculated for each of the three subscales, as well as aggregated to get a full-scale score. The higher the number, the more positive the relationship between the subjects and their parents, as perceived by the student completing the survey. Examples of items from the feelings of affects scale include, “During visits or time spent together, my mother! father was a person I wanted to be with all the time,” “During visits or time spent together, my mother] father was a person towards whom I felt cool and distant,” and “When I go to my mother/ father for help, I continue to feel unsure of myself.” Examples of items from the autonomy scale include, “In general, my mother/ father respect my privacy,” “In general, my mother/ father restrict my freedom or independence,” and “In general, my mother/ father take my opinions seriously.” Examples of items from the emotional support scale include, “Following time spent together, I leave my mother] father with warm and positive feelings,” “Following time spent together, I leave my mother] father feeling let down and disappointed" and “When I have a serious problem or an important decision to make, I know that my mother/ father will know what to do.” The reliability of the attachment measure was assessed by Kenny (1987) through test-retest and internal consistency methods. Test-retest reliability over a 2-week interval was .92 for the measure as a whole and ranged 42 from .82 to .91 for the three scales derived from factor analysis. Cronbach coefficient alpha was calculated for each of the three scales, yielding coefficients of .96, .88, and .88. Internal consistency for the entire measure is Cronbach alpha of .93 for male college students and .95 for female college students. Evidence of construct validity was obtained by correlating each of the three factor scales with subscales of the Moos Family Environment Scale (Moos, 1985). The three factor scales are theoretically consistent with Ainsworth et al.’s (1978) conceptualization of attachment as an enduring affective bond. Inter-Parental Conflict Scale (IPC; Schwarz, 1999) This construct measured the adolescent’s perception of mother’s negative feelings and behaviors regarding father. The Inter-Parental Conflict Scale (IPC) was designed to assess the frequency of overt conflict between spouses. There is a spousal form and a child form, but since this study sample included only children from divorced families, only the child form was used. It contains a list of 34 topics, grouped in four categories: finance and responsibility (6 topics), personal characteristics (13 topics), childrearing practices (12 topics), and family activities (3 topics). College students rated the frequency with which they perceived the mother initiated conflict over each of the 34 topics. IPC items are anchored on a 7-point Likert-type scale, ranging from 0 (never) to 6 (at least once a week). The total IPC score is obtained by taking the mean of numerical rating for all 24 topics. Some sample topics included “insensitivity,” “methods of discipline,” “providing financial assistance,” “punctuality” and “father’s degree of 43 involvement with children.” The internal consistency for ratings by college students is .90 and the one-week test-retest reliability is .86 (Schwarz, 1999). Adolescent Well-being The construct of adolescent well-being is defined as a state of being content and healthy and successful, permitting the experience of consistent, balanced growth. Several dimensions of well-being are involved in human existence, including psychological, spiritual, physical, social, emotional, and intellectual. As stated earlier, within adolescent research, strong emphasis is placed on psychological and spiritual dimensions of well-being (Adams, Bezner, 8 Drabbs, 2000; Adams, Bezner, 8 Steinhardt, 1997; Ardell, 1986; Depken, 1994; Greenburg, 1985; Travis 8 Callander, 1990) in comparison to the other four dimensions (e.g., physical, social, emotional, and intellectual). In this study, thme measures of well-being were utilized in an attempt to capture the full essence of well-being among this college-aged population, therefore more weight was given to the psychological and spiritual dimensions of well-being in the total score of well-being. The standardized scores from the three measures of well- being were added to formulate an aggregated score of well-being. The Perceived Wellness Survey provided an estimate of all six dimensions of well- being and the Current Thoughts Scale provided an estimate of the psychological dimension of well-being. The spiritual dimension was measured using the Personal Meaning Index of the Life Attitude Profile — Revised. The spiritual dimension of well-being has been characterized as “one’s meaning of life” by many researchers (Gatrad, Sadig, 8 Sheikh, 2003; Krippner, 2005; MacKinlay, 44 2001; Paulson, 2005; Schlesing, 2005). Krippner’s (2005) definition of spiritual was “an awareness of a broader life meaning that transcends the immediacy of everyday physical expediency” (p. 225). Likewise, Paulson (2005) defined spirituality as an awareness of a broader life meaning, extending beyond everyday matters and natural concerns. In this study, the spiritual dimension of well-being was defined as a positive sense of meaning and purpose in life. Based on their own belief systems, the students decided individually whether this meaning and purpose in life was fulfilled via mystical peak experiences, connection to other people, linkage to a greater power than the self, or some other source. Cunent Thoughts Scale (C TS; Heatherton 8 Polivy, 1991). This measure of state self-esteem represents the psychological dimension of well-being at the time the scale is completed. Self-esteem was operationalized along a continuous scale indicating the worthiness that the adolescent holds and maintains with regard to him or herself in social, academic, family and personal areas of experience at that particular time. The CTS is a 20- item self-administered inventory assessing attitudes toward the self in social, academic, family, and personal areas of experiences. Scores are indicators of the sense of worthiness that individuals hold toward themselves at that moment. CTS presents respondents with generally favorable (e.g., “I feel confident about my abilities”) or generally unfavorable statements about the self (e.g., “I am worried about what other people think of me”), which they indicate as: not at all (1), a little bit (2), somewhat (3), very much (4), or extremely (5). The CTS is 45 scored as a Likert scale. The original sample for which the scale was developed in 1991 consisted of 428 undergraduates enrolled in Erindale college of the University of Toronto (mean age = 20.3). According to Heatherton and Polivy (1991), the measure of sampling adequacy (MSA) values for each item were found to all be over .80, indicating an acceptable ratio of interitem correlation in partial correlation coefficients (total MSA = .92). The scale also has a high degree of internal consistency (a = .92) and a robust factor structure. Test-retest values ranged from .62 to .71. Personal Meaning Index of the Life Attitude Profile - Revised (LAP-R; Reker, 1992). In agreement with previous researchers, (Adams et al., 2000; Chapman, 1987; Hettler, 1984; Seaward, 1995; Whitmer 8 Sweeney, 1992), the spiritual dimension was defined as a positive sense of meaning and purpose in life. The conceptualization of spiritual well-being in this manner allows students to respond from either a secular or religious belief system. The 48-item LAP-R was designed to measure the multidimensional nature of attitudes toward life. The Personal Meaning Index (PMI) of the Life Attitude Profile — Revised (Reker, 1992) provides a focused measure of personal meaning toward having life goals, a mission or purpose, a sense of direction from past, present, and future, and having a logically integrated and consistent understanding of self, others, and life in general. The PMI is derived by summing the Purpose (PU) and Coherence (CO) dimensions. “Purpose provides thrust and direction to one’s life. Implicit in purpose is the notion of worthwhileness and what is of central importance in a 46 person’s life” (Reker, 1992; p. 14). The subscale contains 8 items scored from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree). Total scores range from 8 to 56, with higher scores indicating a greater life purpose. Sample items include “Basically, I am living the kind of life I want to live,” and “In achieving life’s goals, I have felt completely fulfilled." In the initial psychometric study of the Life Attitude Profile- Revised, the internal consistency for the PU subscale was .86 (Reker, 1992), which compares favorably to the internal consistency found by Adams, Bezner, 8 Drabbs (2000), a = .87. The Coherence dimension is defined as a global orientation that expresses the extent to which one has a pervasive, dynamic feeling of confidence that one’s internal and external environments are predictable and that there is a high probability that things will work out as well as can reasonably be expected. “Implicit in coherence is a sense of order and reason for existence, a clear sense of personal identity, and greater social consciousness” (Reker, 1992; p. 15). The subscale contains 8 items scored from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree. Total scores range from 8 to 56, with higher scores indicating a greater life purpose. Sample items include “I have a philosophy of life that gives my existence significance” and “I have a clear understanding of the ultimate meaning of life.” In the initial psychometric study of the Life Attitude Profile- Revised, the internal consistency for the CO subscale was .82 (Reker, 1992). Perceived Wellness Survey (PWS; Adams 8 Bezner, 1997). Perceived wellness is the sense that one is living in a manner that permits the experience of consistent, balanced growth in the emotional, intellectual, 47 physical, psychological, social, and spiritual dimensions of human existence (Adams 8 Bezner, 1997). Sample items from each dimension are, respectively, "In general, I feel confident about my abilities," "In the past, I have generally found intellectual challenges to be vital to my overall well-being,” ”I expect to always be physically healthy," "In the past, I have expected the best," "My friends will be there for me when I need help," and "I believe there is a real purpose for my life.” Each dimension is represented by 6 items ranging from 1 (very strongly disagree) to 6 (very strongly agree). The dimensional scores are integrated into a wellness composite score by combining the magnitude or mean of each dimension with the balance or standard deviation among dimensions. Scores range from 3 to 29, with higher scores indicating greater wellness. The PWS has been shown to possess very good estimates of factorial and construct validity, as well as internal reliability (a = .91; Adams 8 Bezner, 1997), which compares favorably to the internal consistency found by Adams, Bezner, 8 Drabbs (2000), a = .91. Marlowe-Crowne social desirability scale — M-C form C (Reynolds, 1982). Measures of well-being and reports of parental relationship quality may be confounded by social desirability (i.e., tendencies to answer questions in terms of social appropriateness rather than true well-being), so a social desirability scale was included in the data collection. The Marlowe-Crowne social desirability scale — M-C form C is a 13 item short version of the original 33 item scale. The M-C form C demonstrates an acceptable level of reliability (Kuder-Richardson formula 20 = .76) and compares favorable with the reliability of the standard form 48 (KRzo = .82) and M-C form XX (KRzo = .79), although it has one-third less items than the original form. Concurrent validity was examined via correlations between the Marlowe-Crowne short form and the standard version and the Edwards Social Desirability Scale. The product-moment correlation coefficient with the standard version is .93 and with the Edwards scale it is .41. The correlation with the Edwards scale is consistent with the correlation of .35 found by Crowne and Marlowe (1960). Examples of items from the scale are “I am always courteous, even to people who are disagreeable” and “I have never deliberately said something that hurt someone’s feelings." The shorter form is recommended as a viable short form for use in the assessment of social desirability response tendencies. This form is brief and easy-to-administer. Multigroup Ethnic Identity Measure (MEIM; Phinney, 1992). Measures of well-being and reports of parental relationship quality may be confounded by ethnic identity (i.e., attitudes and knowledge of who one is as a member of an ethnic group), so an ethnic identity measure was included in the data collection. Phinney developed this 12-item, 4-point (1 = strongly disagree, 4 = strongly agree) instrument to address, conceptually and methodologically, ethnic identity as a general phenomenon across groups. The components measured by the MEIM are ethnic affirmation and belonging (feelings of attachment to one's group, ethnic pride, attitudes toward one’s group) and ethnic identity achievement (understanding one’s ethnicity, commitment and secure knowledge of who one is as a member of an ethnic group). The instrument also assesses attitudes toward ethnic groups other than one’s own. Reliability for the 49 MEIM was established using two samples (the largest being high school participants) (Phinney, 1992). Examples of items from the measure are “l have a clear sense of my ethnic background and what it means for me" and “l have spent time trying to find out more about my ethnic group, such as its history, traditions, and customs.” Because the current study involves a university sample, psychometric information for the college sample is reported. The reliability coefficient for the overall measure was .90. Reliability coefficients for the subscales were .86 for Affirrnation and Belonging, .80 for Ethnic Identity Achievement, and .74 for Other-Group Orientation. The MEIM has subsequently been used in dozens of studies and has consistently shown good reliability, typically with alphas above .80 across a wide range of ethnic groups and ages. Design Due to limitations in randomly assigning subjects and in manipulating the predictive variables, a correlational field study was the appropriate design to use in this research endeavor. Determining the relationships among the variables (e.g., the extent the variables are related) and causal modeling are of primary concern in this study. Correlational field designs are known to have good generalizability due to factors such as real life settings and naturally occurring variables. However, due to the lack of control in the experiment, there are potential threats to both internal and external validity. Conclusions drawn from causal modeling with correlational data are valid and unbiased only if the assumed model accurately represents the real causal processes (Tate, 1992). In order to minimize the extraneous error variance, 1) a thorough review of the 50 . iterature was provided, 2) clear operational definitions of each variable was established in accordance with previous research, 3) reliable and valid measures were selected to evaluate each variable, 4) purposive sampling criteria were defined, and 5) the data was analyzed utilizing techniques that best fit the characteristics of the design and data. 08 (“a-Gathering Procedures Participants were recruited via oral and written announcements in classes, organizations, and residential living areas. The recruitment announcement asked students to voluntarily take part in a research project exploring young ad I...- lts’ perceptions of themselves and their parental relationships after the divorce of their parents. Participants completed all questionnaires either ind ividually or within a group setting. At the onset, the informed consent forms were administered and discussed. Subjects were informed that participation was volu ntary and they could withdraw from the study if desired. Volunteers were assured that their privacy would be protected to the maximum extent permissible und er the law. Consistent with the informed consent form, courses of action were discussed to address negative consequences should they present. After the consent forms were collected, each participant was given a packet of the questi<>nnaires, which she or he took home to complete. Completed questi<>nnaires were either returned to this investigator or to the classroom instru ctor to be picked up by this investigator. Each measure within a packet was COded so that the researcher knew which surveys were completed by the same 51 person, but no personally identifiable information was on any of the measures. Packets were returned upon completion. Each participant completed: Personal Data Questionnaire, Parental Attachment Questionnaire (PAQ), Inter-Parental Conflict Scale (IPC), Current Thoughts Scale (CTS), Personal Meaning Index of the Life Attitude Profile — Revised (PMI), Perceived Wellness Survey (PWS), Marlowe-Crowne Social desirability measure (M-C 3), and The Multigroup Ethnic Identity Measure (MEIM). The order of testing for each participant was randomized to control for order effects. Participants had as much time as needed to complete the forms, but total time was approximately 30-40 minutes. The investigator was available to answer questions. Persons who completed all forms had the opportunity to have their names entered into a drawing for one of three $100 awards. Participants who opted to enter the drawing (all except two) provided their email addresses on the informed consent forms. Winners were drawn randomly from the informed consent forms and notified via email. Checks were sent via USPS. Copies of the UCHRIS approval, recruitment announcement, and consent form are provided in Appendix A. Data Analysis Simple correlations and path analysis were appropriate to investigate this study’s specific research questions and hypotheses. Multiple regression was used in performing path analysis. All variables were either continuous or dichotomous, so this type of analysis was the most powerful in determining the causal interrelationships among the variables, (i.e., how each predictor was 52 relating to each other in predicting paternal attachment and well-being). The analysis was completed separately for the two groups: Caucasians (N = 93) as one group and African American (N = 58) and Hispanic Americans (N = 28) as the second group. The data were assessed for significant findings within the groups and between the groups. All model variables were screened for missing data outliers and tested for assumptions. Identification of outliers was done by Conducting a preliminary regression to calculate Mahalanobis distance. The Explore procedure was completed to determine if any cases exceeded the chi square criterion of 16.27 (df =3) for the first path analysis and 24.32 (df =7) for the second path analysis. No outliers were found in either group. Creating scatterplots (see Figures B3, p. 131; C3, p. 140; D3, p. 152; and E3, p. 178) and residuals plots (refer to Figures 82, p. 130; C2, p. 139; DZ, p. 152; and E2, p. 177) assessed test assumptions. The results suggested normality, linearity, and homoscedasticity of the data. Correlation matrices were then created for all the model variables (refer to Tables 4 and 5, p. 64; 8 and 9, p.72) and multicollinearity was evaluated. The presence of minimum and maximum relationship between independent and dependent variables was checked. At least moderately strong relationships were found between the full-scale independent variables and the dependent variables (r > .30). None of the correlations between the full-scale independent variables was greater than .70, so the assumption regarding multicollinearity was not violated. All variables were retained. SPSS also performed collinearity diagnostics on the variables as part of the multiple regression program and eliminated variables 53 accordingly. These analyses indicated that the assumptions of multiple regression have not been violated. No statistical procedure for evaluating the assumptions of path analysis exists (for review of assumptions, refer to Mertler 8 Vannatta, 2003) since they deal specifically with the degree to which the causal model has been correctly specified. Conclusions drawn from causal modeling with correlational data, according to Tate (1998), are confined to the following limitation: the results of causal modeling are valid and unbiased only ifthe assumed model adequately represents the real causal processes. However, there is no empirical test that can evaluate the extent to which the correct model has been selected and described. Tate (1998) suggests that the credibility, reasonableness, and utility of a proposed model be used to address the assumptions regarding correct model specification. In the present study, the extensive literature review, formal theory, personal observations and experiences, and the empirical data all played a substantial role in bringing about the causal specification of the models, and as a whole lend credibility to the results. Research question one was examined using simple correlations for each group (i.e., African American/Hispanic group and Caucasian group; refer to Table 4 and 5, p. 64; 8 and 9, p. 72). The style of maternal attachment was correlabd with the style of paternal attachment by looking at the relationship between full- scale maternal attachment and full-scale paternal attachment, as well as the relationships between each of the attachment subscales (i.e., feelings of affects, parents’ promotions of independence, and parents as sources of support). The 54 degree of maternal conflict with father was then conelated with full-scale paternal attachment and each subscale of paternal attachment. F ull-scale maternal attachment and each subscale of maternal attachment were correlated with adolescent well-being and lastly, full-scale paternal attachment and each subscale of paternal attachment were correlated with adolescent well-being. Adolescent well-being was analyzed using an aggregated score of well-being compiled from the three measures of well-being. The well-being scores were standardized so that the scores were expressed in standard deviations from the mean. The standardized scores from the three measures of well-being were then added to formulate an aggregated score of well-being. Using correlation analysis, ethnic identity and social desirability were found to be covariates within the African American and Hispanic group and social desirability co-varied with the dependent variables within the Caucasian group (refer to tables 6 and 7, p. 67). Regression analyses were conducted to establish the possibility of cause- and-effect relationships among the set of variables that had been logically ordered on the basis of literature and previous research. Using regression analysis in this manner permitted an examination of whether or not the pattern of intercorrelations among the variables fit my underlying theory of which variables were affecting other variables. Specifically, stepwise regression analysis was used to assess how matemal attachment style and maternal conflict with father uniquely influence paternal attachment style and adolescent well-being. Research questions 2, 3, and 4 each include multiple predictor variables and 55 were therefore examined using separate hierarchical MR analyses. The covariates of ethnic identity and social desirability within the African American and Hispanic group and social desirability within the Caucasian group were controlled by putting them in the multiple regression first, as a block. Regression analyses were conducted for the endogenous variables and the full-scale exogenous variables and then with the subscale exogenous variables. Path analyses established the causal ordering of the variables for the African American and Hispanic group and the Caucasian group. Path coefficients for the full-scale variables can be seen in the first two path diagrams (Figure 1, p. 75 and Figure 2, p. 81). For the African American and Hispanic group, coefficients were then used to calculate the reproduced correlations through the path decompositions, which are displayed in Table B, p. 132. Reproduced correlations were calculated and were compared to the empirical correlations (refer to Table 11, p. 78). Only two reproduced correlations exceeded the criterion of a .07 difference. Thus, it was concluded that the revised model was consistent with the empirical data. The final step was to calculate the direct, indirect, and total effects for each endogenous variable (refer to Table 12, p. 78). The path analysis for the Caucasian group resulted in only direct causal effects between the exogenous and endogenous variables and no significant relationship was found between paternal attachment and well-being (Figure 2 and Table 14, p. 81). Consequently, stepwise regression analyses were conducted again using the attachment subscales instead of the full-scale 56 attachment variables (refer to Figure 3, p. 84; Figures D4, 05, and 06, p. 155). Reproduced correlations were calculated through the path decompositions (refer to Table D, p. 153)_and were compared to the empirical correlations (refer to Table 16, p. 85). Only two reproduced correlations exceeded the criterion of a .07 difference. Thus, it was concluded that the revised model was consistent with the empirical data. The next step in this analysis was to calculate the direct, indirect, and total effects for each endogenous variable (refer to Table 17, p. 86). The following step was to construct a path diagram for the African American and Hispanic group using the attachment subscales. Path coefficients for the subscale attachment variables superimposed into one path can be seen in path diagram 4 (p. 91) (refer to E4, E5, E6, pp. 183-184, for the individual paternal attachment subscale paths). Reproduced correlations through the path decompositions were calculated (refer to Table E, p. 179). Reproduced correlations were then compared to the empirical correlations (refer to Table 19, p. 90). The revised model was consistent with the empirical data. The final step was to calculate the direct, indirect, and total effects for each endogenous variable (refer to Table 20, p. 93). Significance Utilization of a modeling process by which covariables were first entered in a stepwise fashion, followed by independent variables allowed for the relative influence of exogenous and endogenous variables to be illuminated. F urtherrnore, this process allowed for an examination of how these influences Changed to examine how these influences changed when other theoretically 57 importar' wastes“.I and prov outcome variation contribu- variance the othe explalmr variables important variables were added to the equation. Significance of relationships was tested using correlations, stepwise multiple regression, and path analysis and provided an index of the relationship between the entire model and the outcome variables. The models convey which variables account for the most variation in the outcome variables, with the Beta weights indicating the relative contribution of each variable to the model. Delta R2 indicates the amount of variance in the outcome variable that the model accounts for when controlling for the other variables and it indicates whether the interaction of the variables is explaining any variance in the dependent variable above and beyond the variables individually. 58 PIBIIITIIF. I D I ”6416' loTableI Conata: | Table 1 m: State 8 Spiral; FLIS . Psyche hgflflepJ " COTTE ' Corr. study, ‘ ThoUgh. Caplure Spiritua; and Pa Pelcelw mdudv “lemme I. As 0" Results Preliminary Analyses Descriptive statistics are summarized in Tables A1, A2, A3, and A4, pp. 116-118. Cronbach’s coefficient alphas are high, ranging from .844 - .957 (refer to Table A5, p. 118). Correlation Analyses Table 1 Correlation Coefficients of Adolescent Well-being Measures Life State Perceived Spiritual Attitude Self- Wellness Dimension Profile Esteem PWS Life Attitudes Survey 1.00 State Self-Esteem .625“ 1.00 Perceived Wellness .698" .698“ 1.00 Spiritual Dimension .774" .646“ .889“ 1.00 PWS Psychological .692” .581 ** .850" .739** Dimension PWS ** Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed) * Correlation is significant at the 0.05 level (2 tailed) Adolescent well-being is the endogenous variable of most interest in this study. Three separate measures of well-being (Life Attitude Profile, Current Thoughts Scale, and Perceived Wellness Survey) were included in an attempt to capture a thorough estimate of the variable. Life Attitude Profile measured spiritual well-being, Current Thoughts Scale measured psychological well-being, and Perceived Wellness Survey provided a general index of well-being. The Perceived Wellness Survey (PWS) addressed six dimensions of well-being, including spiritual wellness and psychological wellness. The correlation coefficients for the three measures and the two dimensions are provided in Table 1 . As can be seen in the table, internal consistency was found among all the 59 ‘—I Table 2 . \. Table 2 African American and Hispanic Group Correlation Coefficients of Well- bein Scores and Independent Variables Aggregated Psychological Spiritual General Well- Well-being Well-being Well-being being Score Score Score Score MIAtt .644“ .575“ .611“ .679** P/Att .581“ .584“ .575" .610“ MICON -.014 -.057 -.105 -.036 MIAFF .612" .612" .639" .702** M/IND .416” .416" .487" .511** MISUP .432" .432** .440" .506“ P/AF F .500“ .500“ .525" .567" P/IND .633" .633" .566" .551" P/SUP .361” .361 ** .378” .432” ETH .488“ .549" .546" .542“ SocD -.543** -.515** -.442** -.501** Maternal Attachment (MlAtt), Maternal conflict with Father (MICON), Paternal Attachment (P/Att), Maternal promotion of independence (MIIND), Maternal feelings of affect (MIAF F), Maternal feelings of support (MSUP), Paternal feelings of affect (P/AF F), Paternal promotion of independence (P/IND), Paternal feelings of support (P/SUP), Ethnic Identity (ETH), Social Desirability (SOCD) *" Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed) “ Correlation is significant at the 0.05 level (2 tailed) Table 3 Caucasian Group Correlation Coefficients of Well-being Scores and Independent Variables Aggregated Psychological Spiritual General Well- Well-being Well-being Well—being being Score Score Score Score MIAtt .399“ .280” .487” .443“ P/Att .350“ .436“ .308“ .505" MICON -.377** -.421** -.266** -.405** M/AF F .351“ .206“ .420** .387" M/l ND .377“ .407** .434” .441“ MISUP .377** .209“ .485“ .404" P/AFF .347" .421" .292" .483“ P/lND .276“ .351 ** .228* .448" P/SUP .333“ .410** .343” .462" ETH .372“ .250” .399” .384" SocD -.074 -.016 -.121 -.157 Maternal Attachment (MlAtt), Maternal conflict with Father (MICON), Paternal Attachment (PIAtt), Maternal promotion of independence (MIIND), Maternal feelings of affect (MIAFF), Maternal feelings of support (MSUP), Paternal feelings of affect (P/AF F ), Paternal promotion of independence (P/IND), Paternal feelings of support (P/SUP), Ethnic Identity (ET H), Social Desirability (SocD) '* Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailad) " Correlation is significant at the 0.05 level (2 tailed) 6O m€BSUI :58.p estima‘ PVIS). psvcbc Include Profile referes Percei TOUT e: and C: measures, as well as between the two measures of psychological well-being (r =.58, p< .01) and the two measures of spiritual well-being (r = .77, p < .01). The well-being scores were standardized and aggregated to form an estimate of spiritual well-being (Life Attitude Profile + spiritual dimension of PWS), an estimate of psychological well-being (Current Thoughts Scale + psychological dimension of PWS), and a total estimate of well-being, which included all three full-scale measures (Current Thoughts Scale + Life Attitude Profile + Perceived Wellness Survey). This total estimate of well-being is merely referenced as “well-being” in the analyses that follow. The scores from the Perceived Wellness Survey are referred to as general well-being. Each of the four estimates of well-being were then correlated with the other study variables and compared (refer to Tables 2 and 3). Consistency was found between the four estimates of well-being and the other variables in this study. If a significant relationship was found with one estimate, it was also found with the other three, and conversely, if an insignificant relationship was found with one estimate, an insignificant relationship was found with the other three estimates. In addition, relationships were consistently either negative or positive across the estimate. Next, the statistical significance of the differences between the correlation coefficients was tested. The r values were converted into standard scores (i.e., z scores) and were then plugged into the following formula to obtain 20b, scores: 20b, = (21 - 22) /the square root of (1/N1 + 1IN2). Within the African American and Hispanic group, no statistically significant differences were found between any of the well-being estimate correlations for 61 119$an differer-I h psyche F 5;; rrl..a regress represe the same variable. Within the Caucasian group, one statistically significant difference was found. This difference was between the correlate for psychological well-being and mother as a source of support and the correlate for spiritual well-being and mother feelings of support, thereby indicating that maternal feelings of support explains significantly more of the variance in spiritual well-being than in psychological well-being. Given that only 1 out of the 132 differences between correlations was significant, the researcher concluded that it would be redundant to run multiple regression with each of the four measures. The aggregated (total) score of well- being appears to be an inclusive and meaningful measure of well-being and represents adolescent well-being in the statistical analyses that follow. These findings indicate that secure attachments to parents are associated with higher levels of general, spiritual, and psychological well-being, while insecure attachments to parents are associated with lower levels of general, spiritual, and psychological well-being, thus supporting hypothesis two. Pearson product-moment correlations among full-scale maternal attachment (MlAtt), full-scale paternal attachment (PIAtt), maternal conflict with father (MICON) and well-being (WB) are shown in Tables 4 and 5 (p. 64). Consistent with prior research (e.g., Carranza 8 Kilmann, 2000; Hojat, 1998; McCormick 8 Kennedy, 2000; Meyers, 1998), maternal and paternal attachment were found to have a strong positive correlation with well-being within the African American and Hispanic group (r = 0.64, p < .01 and r = 0.58, p < .05, respectively) and a moderately strong positive correlation within the Caucasian 62 grour with : of se posit mall rest corr 1686 R05 deg group (r = .40, p < .01 and r = .35, p < .01, respectively). Therefore, students with a more secure attachment style with either parent depicted a greater degree of sense of well-being. As hypothesized, paternal style of attachment was positively correlated with maternal style of attachment within both groups (r = 0.34, p < .01 and r = .23, p < .05, respectively) and negatively correlated with maternal conflict with father (r = -0.23, p < .05 and r = -0.31, p < .01, respectively). However, maternal style of attachment was not significantly correlated with maternal conflict with father, a finding that differs from prior research (e.g., Amato, 1993; Emery 8 Forehand, 1994; Lewis, Fairing, 8 Rosenthal, 2000; Summers et.al., 1998; Whiteside 8 Becker, 2000). In summary, within both groups, there is a significant relationship between a) the style of maternal attachment and the style of paternal attachment, b) the degree of maternal conflict with father and the style of paternal attachment, c) the style of maternal attachment and adolescent well-being, and d) the style of paternal attachment and adolescent well-being. These findings support hypothesis one for both groups, that is, secure attachment to mother and low levels of maternal conflict with father correspond with secure attachment to father. Conversely, insecure attachment to mother and high levels of maternal conflict with father correspond with insecure attachment to father. 63 Sir: Wit bet Arr TE}; re: be an Table 4 African American and Hispanic Group Correlations of Main Variables MIAtt MICON P.Att WB MIAtt 1 .00 MICON -.187 1.00 P/Att .336” -.228* 1 .00 WB .644“ -.014 .581* 1.00 Maternal Attachment (MlAtt), Maternal conflict with Father (MICON), Paternal Attachment (PIAtt), Well-being (WB) ** Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed) * Correlation is significant at the 0.05 level (2 tailed) Table 5 Caucasian Group Correlations of Main Variables MIAtt MICON P.Att WB MIAtt 1 .00 MICON -.040 1 .00 PlAtt .232" -.314** 1 .00 WB .399“ -.377** .350" 1 .00 Maternal Attachment (MlAtt), Maternal conflict with Father (MICON), Paternal Attachment (PIAtt), Well-being (WB) ““ Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed) “ Correlation is significant at the 0.05 level (2 tailed) Hypothesis two was also supported. Secure parental attachments had strong positive correlations with general, spiritual and psychological well-being. Within the Caucasian group, a moderately strong positive relation was found between maternal conflict with father and well-being, but within the African American and Hispanic group, the data indicated there was not a significant relationship. Thus, secure attachment to mother corresponds with secure attachment to father, yet interparental discord does not appear to influence the well-being of the child. While research supports negative impact of interparental conflict on adolescent well-being, it is important to remember that the majority of research has been done using predominantly Caucasian samples. A different pattern may exist with Hispanic and African American adolescents than is found among Caucasian students, contrary to hypothesis 3. Given these results and the fr relate being deta Inter for s lnvo and varrr pate are for l tabl the finding that maternal attachment and maternal conflict with father were related to paternal attachment, but not to each other, nor consistently to well- being (even though paternal attachment was related to well-being), a more detailed analysis of the data was warranted. In order to control unwanted variance that may be affecting or biasing the interpretations of the data, Pearson product-moment correlations were generated for several factors that have been identified as covariants in previous studies involving parental attachment and divorce. These factors are listed in Table 6 and 7, p. 67. The correlations between these factors and the dependent variables of this study (e.g., parental attachment, parental feelings of affect, paternal promotion of independence, father feelings of support, and well-being), are found in Table 6 for the African American and Hispanic group and in Table 7 for the Caucasian group. Age at time of separation was not included in the tables because it was not significantly different than age at time of divorce (Cohen’s D = .016, effect size = .008) and 57 students failed to provide an age (37% of the African American and Hispanic group and 27% of the Caucasian group). In examining Table 6, two variables stand out as significant covariates, specifically, ethnic identity and social desirability. Both ethnic identity and social desirability had a strong relationship with well-being (r = 0.49 and —0.54, respectively, p < .01) and were treated as covariates in further analysis of the data. Ethnic identity also correlated with full-scale paternal attachment (r = 0.33, p < .05) and the three paternal attachment subscales (r = 0.33, 0.26, 0.22, p < .05, respectively), with the strength of the relationships ranging from small to 65 rnedr <.OE depe appe aux: cone they nfiat rnot 02D 025 Gee medium. Social desirability correlated with paternal feelings of affect (r = -.023, p < .05) and paternal promotion of independence (r = -0.24, p < .05). As is evident in Table 6, several other factors co-varied with the dependent variables of this study. While most of these significant relationships appear commonsensical (i.e., as students grow older they experience more autonomy-promoting behaviors from their fathers), the majority have small correlations (e.g., r = .10 - .29, Cohen, 1988), suggesting weak relationships and they were not included in the regression analysis as covariates. Even though the relationships were week, the significant positive relationships between 1) mother’s education level and the paternal attachment subscales (r = 0.25, 0.31, 0.27; p < .05) and 2) adolescent well-being and mother’s education level (r = 0.25, p < .05), father’s education level (r = 0.29, p < .01), and the student’s education level (r = 0.22, p < .05) are quite interesting and were addressed in the discussion section of this paper. 66 Table 6 African American and Hispanic Group Pearson Correlation Coefficients of Endogenous Variables and Possible Covariates P/Att P/AFF P/IND PISUP WB _Age .142 .091 .267* .072 .267* Education Level .083 .052 .167 .036 .217* _Age at Divorce 253* 261* .141 .276" .159 Lengm parents Committed Rel. -.025 .062 -.O77 -.058 .007 No. of Siblings with same parents .078 .146 .031 .024 .073 Freq maternal contact currently -.172 -.093 -.258* -.069 -.400** Frequency maternal contact < 18 .071 .077 .131 .009 .017 Freq paternal contact currently -.461** -.481** -.204 -.454** -.127 Frequency paternal contact < 18 -.281** -.341** .142 -.467** .228* Mother’s education level 247* 313* .003 275* 250* Father’s education level .017 .118 -.028 .045 .288“ Ethnic Identity .327" .330“ 256* 216* .488" Social Desirability -.182 -.233* -.243* .022 -.543** Paternal Attachment (PIAtt), Paternal feelings of affect (P/AF F ), Paternal promotion of independence (PlIND), Father feelings of support ((PISUP), Total Wellobeing measure (WB) *" Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed) * Correlation is significant at the 0.06 level (2 tailed) Table 7 Caucasian Group Pearson Correlation Coefficients of Endgqenous Variables and Possible Covariates P/Att P/AF F P/IND PISUP WB Age .161 .146 .122 .166 .170 Education Level .027 .004 .053 .026 .127 Age at Divorce -.017 -.052 -.149 .135 -.207* Length parents Committed Rel. -.002 -.043 -.113 .136 -.199 No. of Siblings with same parents -.069 -.078 -.252* .105 -.163 Freq maternal contact currently .005 .001 -.022 .025 -.241* Frequency maternal contact < 18 .104 .101 .006 .150 -.021 Freq paternal contact currently -.503** -.478** -.171 -.611** -.257* Frequency paternal contact < 18 -.497** -.409** -.251*. -.663** -.011 Mother’s education level .100 .136 .071 .030 230* Father’s education level .105 .117 -.057 .176 -.005 Ethnic ldentig .145 .131 .053 .203 .372" Social Desirability -.304** -.326** -.178 -.297** -.074 Paternal Attachment (PIAtt), Paternal feelings of affect (P/AF F), Paternal promotion of independence (P/IND), Father feelings of support ((PISUP), Total Well-being measure (WB) ** Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed) * Correlation is significant at the 0.06 level (2 tailed) 67 frequr morit Liker betwi there more are r 1981 visit the mor atta evlc onl aft; Of : thr wit ail ml It is important to note that the possible choices to questions about frequency of contact with parent (e.g., mother or father) included daily, weekly, monthly, couple times a year, and not at all. The responses were scored on a Likert scale ranging from 1 to 5, with 1 being daily. The negative correlations between frequency of contact and attachment subscales and well-being are therefore interpreted as higher levels of contact with parent are associated with more secure attachment styles and/or more positive well-being. These findings are consistent with prior research (Hetherington, et al., 1982; Wallerstein 8 Kelly, 1980). Several of these correlations involving frequency of contact with parent indicate a moderately strong relationship (r = .30 - .49, Cohen, 1988), but given the nominal nature of the data and the logical structure of the relationships (i.e., more contact, more secure attachment style, or vice versa, more secure attachment style, more contact), these correlations served as supportive evidence to other findings, but were not included in the regression analysis. In examining the possible covariates with the Caucasian group (Table 7), only social desirability stood out as a significant covariate with paternal attachment (r = -0.30, p < .01) and the paternal attachment subscales of feelings of affect (r = -0.33, p < .01) and father feelings of support (r = -0.30, p < .01). All three were moderately strong relationships. Ethnic identity neither correlated with full-scale paternal attachment, nor any of the subscales of paternal attachment, yet it did have a moderately strong correlation with well-being and was thus evaluated for covariance in the regression analysis. Using stepwise multiple regression, ethnic identity did not account for a significant amount of the 68 vanar vafial exha sgni weal cont anal (Zau dep vwel lNhl Io variance in well-being within the Caucasian group when the other independent variables were controlled. The Caucasian group did not significantly correlate with as many extraneous variables as the African American and Hispanic group. The significant relationships (refer to Table 7, p. 67) not already mentioned, were weak (r = .10 -.29) or were between the dependent variables and frequency of contact with parent. Again, these variables were not included in the regression analysis for the reasons stated previously. It is interesting to note that within the Caucasian group, the only significant correlation between education level and the dependent variables was found between mother’s education and adolescent well-being (r = 0.23, p < .05), unlike the African American and Hispanic group in which several significant relationships were related to education level. To summarize the covariate data, ethnic identity and social desirability were significant covariates within the African American and Hispanic group and needed to be entered first when doing regression analysis. Within the Caucasian group, social desirability significantly correlated with the dependent variables and was thus entered first when doing multiple regression analysis. In addition, correlates of frequency of parental contact with the dependent variables promoted the idea that more secure attachment styles and more contact with parents go hand in hand. Table 8 and Table 9 (p. 72) present the Pearson product-moment correlations among maternal conflict with father, well-being, and the subscales for maternal and paternal attachment. Within the Caucasian group, internal 69 consistency was found among the subscales for paternal attachment, (r = 0.75, 0.82, 0.53, p < .01), and among the subscales for maternal attachment (r = 0.64, 0.89, 0.52, p < .01). Within the African American and Hispanic group, internal consistency was found between paternal feelings of affect and both paternal promotion of independence (r = 0.54, p < .01) and father feelings of support (r = 0.88, p < .01), but only a moderate correlation was found between father feelings of support and paternal promotion of independence (r = 0.38, p < .01). A similar pattern was found among the maternal subscales in this group (r = 0.60, 0.84, and 0.30, respectively, p < .01). This pattern was evaluated later in light of additional analysis. Examining the correspondence between secure attachment to mother with secure attachment to father within the African American and Hispanic group, six of the nine correlations among the subscales were significant. Paternal promotion of independence positively correlated with maternal feelings of affect, maternal promotion of independence, and maternal feelings of support (r = .43, p < .01; r = 0.40, p < .01; and r = 0.23, p < .05 respectively). Paternal feelings of affect positively correlated with maternal feelings of affect and maternal promotion of independence (r = 0.25 and 0.27, p < .01, respectively). Paternal feelings of support only correlated with maternal promotion of independence (r = .30, p < .01). Note that the strength of the correlation is similar to that found between paternal feelings of support and paternal promotion of independence (r = .38, p < .01) and a similar finding was found with the maternal subscales, that 70 is, maternal melings of support only correlated with the paternal promotion of independence within the paternal attachment subscales (r = .23, p < .01). Examining the variable maternal conflict with father, a significant negative relationship was found with maternal feelings of support (r = -0.24, p < .05), paternal feelings of affect (r = -0.29, p < .01), and paternal feelings of support (r = -0.26, p < .05). All six attachment subscales were positively related to adolescent well-being, four strong (r = 0.67, 0.51, 0.51, 0.61, p < .01) and two moderately strong (r = 0.48 and 0.38, p < .01). Despite these correlations, no relationship was found between maternal conflict with father and well-being and between maternal feelings of support and paternal feelings of support. Examining the correspondence between secure attachment to mother with secure attachment to father within the Caucasian group, four of the nine correlations among the subscales were significant. Paternal promotion of independence positively correlated with maternal feelings of affect and maternal promotion of independence (r = 0.32 and 0.32 respectively, p < .01). Paternal feelings of affect correlated weakly with only maternal feelings of affect (r = 0.21, p < .05). Paternal feelings of support correlated with only maternal promotion of independence (r = .32, p < .01) (as was the case in the African American and Hispanic group). Maternal feelings of affect did not correlate with any of the paternal attachment subscales. Examining the variable maternal conflict with father, a significant negative relationship was found with all three of the paternal attachment subscales (r = -0.24, p < .05, r = -0.38 and —0.30, p< .01). 71 Table 8 Pearson Correlation Coefficients African American and Hispanic Group Attachment Subscales Well-bein , and Maternal conflict with Father MIAFF MIIND MISUP PIAFF PIIND PISUP WB M/AF F 1.00 MIIND .598“ 1.00 MISUP .843" .304“ 1 .00 P/AF F .250* .271 * .142 1.00 PIIND .425” .404" .231 * .542" 1.00 PISUP .108 .299“ .032 .878“ .381 ** 1 .00 WB .672” .506“ .476“ .508“ .612** .378“ 1 .00 MICON -.088 -.172 -.235* -.293** -.004 -.255* -.014 Maternal promotion of independence (MIIND), Maternal feelings of affect (M/AF F), Maternal feelings of support (MSUP), Paternal feelings of affect (PIAF F), Paternal promotion of independence (PIIND), Paternal feelings of support (PISUP), Well-being (WB), Ethnic Identity (ETH), Social Desirability (SocD) “ Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed) ' Correlation is significant at the 0.05 level (2 tailed) Table 9 Pearson Correlation Coefficients Caucasian Group Attachment Subscales, Well-being and Maternal conflict with Father M/AFF MIIND MISUP PIAFF PIIND PISUP WB M/AF F 1.00 MIIND .640" 1.00 MISUP .885" .522" 1 .00 P/AFF .211* .181 .107 1.00 P/IND .305" .317" .146 .751" 1.00 PISUP .180 .317" .146 .824“ .533" 1.00 WB .351“ .377" .377" .347" .276“ .333“ 1.00 MICON -.022 -.195 .069 -.238* -.377** -.295** -.377** Maternal promotion of independence (MIIND), Maternal feelings of affect ( M/AF F), Maternal feelings of support (MSUP), Paternal feelings of affect (PIAF F), Paternal promotion of independence (PIIND), Paternal feelings of support (PISUP), Well-being (WB), Ethnic Identity (ET H), Social Desirability (SocD) ** Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed) ” Correlation is significant at the 0.05 level (2 tailed) 72 Unlike the African American and Hispanic group, within the Caucasian group, a moderately strong negative relationship was found between maternal conflict with father and adolescent well-being (r = -0.38, p < .01). All six attachment subscales had moderately strong positive relationships with adolescent well-being (r = 0.35, 0.38, 0.38, 0.35, 0.28, 0.34, p < .01), somewhat weaker relationships than found in the African American and Hispanic group. No relationship was found between maternal conflict with father and the maternal attachment subscales, thereby indicating that interparental conflict, even when perceived to be instigated by the mother, does not appear to influence 1) maternal attachment style nor 2) secure maternal attachment impact on Caucasian adolescent well-being. To summarize Tables 8 and 9, within the African American and Hispanic group, there was a significant relationship between a) the degree of maternal conflict with father and paternal feelings of affect and support, b) the subscales of maternal attachment and adolescents well-being, and c) the subscales of paternal attachment and adolescents well-being. Six of the nine correlations between maternal attachment subscales and paternal attachment subscales were significant. These findings support hypothesis one, that is, secure attachment to mother and low levels of maternal conflict with father correspond with secure attachment to father. Conversely, insecure attachment to mother and high levels of maternal conflict with father correspond with insecure attachment to father. These findings suggest more specificity is warranted in defining the relationship between maternal attachment, paternal attachment, and 73 maternal conflict with father. The paternal attachment subscales appear to be affected differently by the different maternal attachment subscales and by maternal conflict with father. These finding also support hypothesis two, namely, secure attachments to parents are associated with higher levels of adolescent well-being and insecure attachments to parents are associated with lower levels of adolescent well-being. The findings suggest that the strength of some subscales of attachment is more or less than that of other subscales in impacting adolescent well-being. Within the Caucasian group, there is a significant relationship between a) the degree of maternal conflict with father and paternal feelings of affect and support, b) the subscales of maternal attachment and adolescent well-being, and c) the subscales of paternal attachment and adolescent well-being. Four of the nine correlations between maternal attachment subscales and paternal attachment subscales are significant. These findings support hypothesis 1, but suggest specific subscales of maternal attachment and maternal conflict affect specific subscales of paternal attachment. In addition, the differences between the two groups illustrated by these correlations suggest the hypothesis (3.a) that Hispanic and African American adolescents’ parental attachment patterns are the same as Caucasian adolescents’ parental attachment patterns may be inaccurate. On the other hand, these differences do lend support to the hypothesis (3.b) that Hispanic and African American adolescents possess more adaptive coping strategies in relation to maternal conflict with father than do Caucasian adolescents. Definitive conclusions require further analyses. 74 Path Analysis Table 10 African American and Hispanic Group Pearson Correlation Coefficients Endogenous and Exogenous Variables Used in Path 1 MIAtt M/Con P/Att WB ETH M/Att 1 .00 MICON -.187 1.00 P/At’t .336* -.228* 1 .00 WB .644" -.014 .581“ 1.00 ETH .054 .147 .327" .488" 1 .00 $060 -.435** .243* -.182 -.543** -.053 Maternal Attachment (MlAtt), Maternal conflict with Father (MICON), Paternal Attachment (PIAtt), Well-being (WB), Ethnic Identity (ETH), Social Desirability (SocD). ** Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed) * Correlation is significant at the 0.05 level (2 tailed) 3 Social Desirability -.435" l Maternal Attachment w .336" 2 Maternal conflict 4 Paternal with Father @ Attachment Figure 1 Analysis 1 75 F Well- being 5 Ethnic Identity Input Path Diagram 1: African American] Hispanic Group Path Path analysis was completed alter identifying the covariates and exploring the correlates between the independent and dependent variables. Straight, single-headed arrows, representing unidirectional paths, connect the boxes. These straight arrows originate at the variable exerting the influence and point towards the variable being affected. Curved, two headed arrows represent correlations found between exogenous variables. The standardized path coefficients (b) represent the effect of a given predictor variable on the dependent variable after accounting for the remaining relationships in the model. The variables and Pearson product-moment correlations for this analysis are found in Table 10. The covariates of ethnic identity and social desirability were included in the model. Figure 1 illustrates the first path analysis for the African American and Hispanic group. Path decomposition was used to examine goodness-of-fit. This process involved the identification of all legitimate paths between the variables in the model, resulting in a correlation coefficient equal to the product of all coefficients in that path. Correlational decompositions were determined for all possible bivariate correlations in the models, with the exception of those between exogenous variables. The decompositions and calculations of reproduced correlations for this path analysis can be found in Table 81 (p. 132). The reproduced correlations are displayed adjacent to the observed correlations in Table 11 (p. 78). Calculation of reproduced correlations through path decompositions and subsequent comparison to the empirical correlations indicated the model fits the empirical data. 76 For the African American and Hispanic group, a path analysis was conducted to determine the causal effects among the variables maternal attachment (MlAtt), maternal conflict with father (MICON), paternal attachment, (PIAtt), and adolescent well-being (WB). Prior to the analysis, two covariates were identified (Table 6, p. 67) and added to the analysis, specifically, ethnic identity (ETH) and social desirability (SocD). Stepwise multiple regression was used to establish the causal effect relationships (refer to Figure 81, p. 120). The model was also tested without the inclusion of the ethnic identity and social desirability and the results did not fit the model (e.g., MICON was positively correlated with WB and MICON was not correlated with PlAtt). All path coefficients were significant at or below the .05 level. Utilizing calculations from Table B (p. 132), the direct, indirect and total causal effects of the model are presented in Table 12, p. 78. R2 is noted for each endogenous variable. The outcome of primary interest was adolescent well-being; the determinant with the largest total causal effect was maternal attachment (.693). The remaining determinants of adolescent well-being as indicated by the total causal effect were paternal attachment (.523), social desirability (-.331), ethnic identity (.320), and maternal conflict with father (-.096). This model explained approximately 78% of variance in adolescent well-being. The primary determinant of paternal attachment was maternal attachment (.336) with maternal conflict with father (- .228) following. Approximately 26% of the variance in paternal attachment was explained by the model. The primary determinant of ethnic identity was paternal attachment (.327), followed by maternal attachment (.110) and maternal conflict 77 Table 11 African American and Hispanic Group Path 1 Observed and Reproduced Correlations (Table 31) M/A'I'l" MICON P/ATT WB ETH Observed Correlations MIATT 1.00 MICON -.187 1 .00 PIATI' .336 -.228 1 .00 WB .644 -.014 .581 1.00 ETH .054 .147 .327 .488 1 .00 SocD -.435 .243 -.182 -.543 -.053 Reproduced Correlations MIATI' 1.00 MICON - 1.00 P/A'l‘l' .336 -.228 1 .00 WB .693 .096* .608 1 .00 ETH .109 -.075 .327 .424 1.00 SocD -.435 - -.146 -.393* -.048 *Difference >.07 Table 12 Summery for Causal Effects for African American and Hispanic Group Path 1 flcome Determinant Direct Indirect TLal PlAtt MIAtt 336* -- .336 (R2 = .257) MICON -.228* —— -.228 we M/Att .407" .286 .693 (R2 = .780) MICON --- -.096 -.096 PlAtt .318“ .105 .523 ETH .320" —— .320 $000 -.331** -- -.331 ETH PlAtt .327** —— .327 (R2 = .107) MIAtt -— .110 .110 MICON —- -.075 -.075 SocD MIAtt -.435** ~— -.435 (R2 = .189) ** Direct effect is significant at the .01 level. * Direct effect is significant at the .05 level. 78 with father (-.O75). Approximately 11% of variance in ethnic identity was explained by the model. The primary determinant of social desirability was maternal attachment (—.435), which explained approximately 19% of the variance in social desirability. To summarize, among African American and Hispanic students, secure maternal attachment and low levels of maternal conflict with father had a direct causal influence on secure paternal attachment. This means that when the style of maternal attachment was controlled, the degree of maternal conflict with father uniquely predicted the style of paternal attachment. In addition, through its impact on paternal attachment, maternal attachment had an indirect impact on adolescent well-being, in addition to its direct influence. Maternal attachment did not correlate with maternal conflict with father, which means the style of maternal attachment did not moderate the relationship between the degree of maternal conflict with father and the style of paternal attachment. However, both maternal attachment style and maternal conflict with father uniquely predicted the relationship between the style of paternal attachment and adolescent well-being (i.e., in addition to its direct influence, paternal attachment had an indirect influence on well-being stemming from the impact of maternal attachment and maternal conflict with father). This path analysis also indicated that students with secure maternal attachment tend to have more of an attitude of openness and acceptance of shortcomings (i.e., maternal attachments’ impact on social desirability) than students with insecure maternal attachment. Paternal 79 all; he 81 attachment has a moderately strong influence on ethnic identity, which in turn, has a moderately strong influence on adolescent well-being. The first path analysis for the Caucasian group is presented in Figure 2, p. 81. Straight, single-headed arrows, representing unidirectional paths, connect the variables. These straight arrows originate at the variable exerting the influence and point towards the variable being affected. As illustrated, this path diagram consists of only bivariate connections, that is, a variable is either endogenous or exogenous, and so no path consists of more than two variables. There are also no correlations found between the exogenous variables (i.e., maternal attachment, maternal conflict with father, and social desirability), thus there are no curved, two headed arrows present. The standardized path coefficients (b) represent the effect of a given predictor variable on the dependent variable after accounting for the remaining relationships in the model. The variables and Pearson product-moment correlations for this analysis are found in Table 13, p. 81. The covariate social desirability was identified previously and was included in the model. 80 Table 13 Caucasian Group Pearson Correlation Coefficients Endogenous and Exogenous Variables Used in Path 1 MIAtt M/Con PlAtt WB MIAtt 1 .00 MICON -.O40 1 .00 PlAtt 232* -.314** 1 .00 WB .399“ -.377** .350" 1 .00 8060 -.037 -.O74 -.304** -.074 Maternal Attachment (MIATT), Maternal conflict with Father (MICON), Paternal Attachment (PlAtt), Well-being (WB), Ethnic Identity (ET H), Social Desirability (SocD). ** Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed) ‘ Correlation is significant at the 0.05 level (2 tailed) 5 Paternal Attachment 0 l Maternal Attachment 2 Maternal Conflict ’ Well- being 3 Social Desirability Figure 2 Input Path Diagram 2: Caucasian Group Path Analysis 1 Table 14 Caucasian Group Path 1 Observed Correlations and Standardized Path Coefficients M/Att M/CON SocD Observed r PlAtt 0.232 -0.314 -0.304 WB 0.399 -0.377 Standardized b PlAtt 0.231 -0.338 -0.329 WB 0.385 -0.362 0.000 Maternal Attachment (MlAtt), Paternal Attachment (PlAtt), Maternal conflict with father (MICON), Social Desirability (SocD), Well-being (WB). ‘Indicates differences > .05. 81 Figure 2 illustrates the first path analysis for the Caucasian group. Path decomposition is typically used to examine goodness—of-fit. This process involves the identification of all legitimate paths between the variables in the model, resulting in a correlation coefficient equal to the product of all coefficients in the path. However, in this path analysis, all legitimate paths are only one-way, bivariate paths. Each variable is either endogenous or exogenous and there are no significant relationships between the exogenous variables. Therefore, the standardized path coefficient (b) equates to the product of all coefficients in that path. This means the standardized path coefficients were compared to the observed correlations (r) and are displayed in Table 14. Comparison of the coefficients in the table indicated no differences between observed correlation and standardized path correlation exceeded the .05 level, except for paternal attachment and adolescent well-being. The correlational analysis indicated a moderately strong correlation (r = .35, 0< .01) between paternal attachment and adolescent well-being. This relationship was fundamental to the current study and was lost during the stepwise multiple regression (Figure C1, p. 134) once maternal attachment and maternal conflict with father were controlled. Therefore, this model did not fit the empirical data and further analysis was warranted. The correlational analysis of the attachment subscales (refer to Tables 8 and 9, p. 72) indicated that there were significant differences in how the subscales related to each other and to the other variable of this study (e.g., maternal conflict with father, adolescent well-being, ethnic identity, and social 82 desirability). These findings prompted the path analysis to be revised so that the attachment subscales were used in place of the full-scale measures. This analysis was completed for both groups. The variables and Pearson product- moment correlations for the Caucasian group analysis are found in Table 15, p. 84. The covariate of social desirability was included in the model, along with the main exogenous and endogenous variables. Figure 3 is the second path analysis for the Caucasian group. It illustrates the superimposed path analyses of the three paternal subscales. The separate paths for each of the paternal subscales are found in Figures D4, D5, and D6, p. 155. Path decomposition was used to examine goodness-of-fit. All legitimate paths between the variables in the model were identified and resulted in a correlation coefficient for each path, which equals the product of all coefficients in the path. Correlational decompositions were determined for all possible bivariate correlations in the models, with the exception of those between exogenous variables. The decompositions and calculations of reproduced correlations for this path analysis can be found in Table D, p. 153. The reproduced correlations are displayed adjacent to the observed correlations in Table 16, p. 85. Comparison of reproduced correlations and empirical correlations indicated the model fit the empirical data. 83 Table 15 Pearson Correlation Coefficients Caucasian Group Endogenous and Exogenous Variables Used in Path 2 MIAFF MIIND MISUP P/AF F PIIND PISUP WB ETH SocD M/AF F 1 .00 MIIND .640" 1 .00 MISUP .885“ .522“ 1 .00 PIAFF .211* .181 .107 1.00 PIIND .305” .317" .146 .751” 1.00 PISUP .180 .31 7** . 146 .824" .533" 1 .00 WB .351” .377“ .377“ .347” .276“ .333“ 1 .00 ETH .384“ .252" .51 2" .1 31 .053 .203 .372” 1 .00 SocD -.002 .054 .064 -.326** -.178 -.297** -.074 -.068 1.00 MICON -.022 -.195 .069 -.238* -.377** -.295** -.377** -.096 -.074 Maternal promotion of independence (MIIND), Maternal feelings of affect (MIAFF), Maternal feelings of support (MSUP), Paternal feelings of affect (PIAFF), Paternal promotion of independence (PIIND), Paternal feelings of support (PISUP), Well-being (WB), Ethnic Identity (ET H), Social Desirability (8000), Maternal conflict with father (MICON) ** Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed) * Conelation is significant at the 0.05 level (2 tailed) MISUP M/AF F MICON .685“ .205“ Soc/D -.345" -.437" Q .259“ Q -.332* -.265* .283‘ PIIND 7 .377“ P/AFF 6 Well- being 9 -.350" PISUP 8 125 = .885 r23 = .640 r35 = .522 Figure 3 Input Path Diagram 3: Caucasian Group Path Analysis 2 84 Table 16 Caucasian Group Path 2 Observed and Reproduced Correlations MIAFF M/er MISUP MICON Observed Correlations Pimp P/gspp PIAFF W48, MIAFF 1.00 MIIND .640 1.00 MISUP .885 .522 1 .00 MICON -.022 -.195 .069 1.00 PIIND .305 .317 .146 -.377 1.00 PISUP .180 .317 .146 -.295 .533 1.00 PIAFF .211 .181 .107 -.238 .751 .824 1.00 WB .351 .377 .377 -.377 .276 .333 .347 SocD -.002 .054 .064 -.074 -.178 -.297 -.326 -.074 Reproduced Correlations MIAFF 1.00 MIIND — 1.00 MISUP - - 1.00 MICON - - - 1.00 PIIND .298 .211* .169 -.332 1.00 PISUP .181 .283 .148 -.265 - 1.00 P/AF F .205 .131 .181 -.259 -- - 1.00 WB .380 .226* .378 -.408 .230 .280 .339 SocD - -— - - - -.33; -.345 -.077 *Difference >07 85 Table 17 Summem for Causal Effects for Caucasian Group Path 2 Outcome D_et_ermina_nt [_)i_@ct Ingrid Tot_a_l PIAFF MISUP .— .... ....+ (R2 = .217) MICON -.259* .... -259 MIAFF 205* _ .205 MIIND .... __ __ + $000 -.345** -- -.345 PISUP MISUP .... .... .. + (R2 + .262) MICON -.265* _.. -265 MIAFF __ __ __ + MIIND 283* -- .283 SocD -.332* -- -.332 PIIND MISUP -.437* -- -.437 * (r?2 = .270) MICON -.332* .... -332 MIAFF .685* __ __+ MIIND .... .... __ + we MISUP .377** --- .377 * (R2: .352) MICON -.350** -.058 -.408 MIAFF .— .046 .046 * MIIND .... __ _ + PIAFF .224 .... .224 SocD —- -.077 -.077 “Direct effect is significant at he .01 level. *Direct effect is significant at the .05 level. * Total effect my be incomplete due to unanalyzed components For the Caucasian group, a path analysis was conducted to determine the causal effects among the variables Maternal promotion of independence (MIIND), Maternal feelings of affect (M/AF F), Maternal feelings of support (MSUP), Maternal conflict with father (MICON), Paternal feelings of affect (PIAF F), Paternal promotion of independence (PIIND), Paternal feelings of support (PISUP), Well-being (WB), and Social Desirability (SocD). Stepwise multiple regression was used to establish the cause-and-effect relationships (refer to Figure D1, p. 142). All path coefficients were significant at or below the 86 .05 level. Recall that in the first path analysis, the relationship between paternal attachment and adolescent well-being was no longer significant once maternal attachment and maternal control toward father were controlled. However, in the correlational analysis, PlAtt and WB positively correlated (r = .35, p< .01). In this analysis, a direct relationship was found between paternal feelings of affect and well-being (r = .224, p < .05), but not between paternal feelings of support or paternal promotion of independence and adolescent well-being. Utilizing calculations from Table D (p. 153), the direct, indirect and total causal effects of the model are presented in Table 17. R2 is noted for each endogenous variable. Once again, the outcome of primary interest was adolescent well-being; the determinant with the largest total causal effect was maternal conflict with father (-.408). The remaining determinants of adolescent well-being as indicated by the total causal effect were maternal feelings of support (.377), and paternal feelings of affect (.224), social desirability (-.077) and maternal feelings of affect (.046). This model explained approximately 35% of variance in adolescent well-being. The primary determinant of paternal feelings of affect was social desirability (-.345) with maternal conflict with father (- .259) and maternal feelings of affect (.205) following. Approximately 22% of the variance in paternal feelings of affect was explained by the model. The primary determinant of paternal feelings of support was social desirability (-.332), followed by maternal promotion of independence (.283) and maternal conflict with father (-.265). Approximately 26% of the variance in paternal feelings of support was explained by the model. The primary determinant of paternal 87 promotion of independence was maternal feelings of support (-.437), followed by maternal conflict with father (-.332). Note the negative correlation between paternal promotion of independence and maternal feelings of support. Approximately 27% of variance in paternal promotion of independence was explained by the model. The second Path analysis for the African American and Hispanic group was completed, with the attachment subscales used in place of the full-scale attachment measures. The variables and Pearson product-moment correlations for the Caucasian group analysis are found in Table 18, p. 90. The covariates of ethnic identity and social desirability were included in the model, along with the exogenous and endogenous variables. Figure 4, p. 91, is the second path analysis for the African American and Hispanic group. It illustrates the superimposed path analyses of the three paternal subscales. The separate paths for each of the paternal subscales are found in Figures E4, E5, and E6, pp. 183-184. Path decomposition was used to examine goodness-of-fit. All legitimate paths between the variables in the model were identified and resulted in a correlation coefficient for each path. Correlation decompositions were determined for all possible bivariate correlations in the models, with the exception of those between exogenous variables. The decompositions and reproduced correlations for can be found in Table E, p. 179. The reproduced correlations are displayed adjacent to the observed correlations in Table 19 (p. 90). The comparison of reproduced correlations and empirical correlations indicated the model fit the empirical data. 88 For the African American and Hispanic group, a path analysis was conducted to determine the causal effects among the variables Maternal promotion of independence (MIIND), Maternal feelings of affect (MIAF F), Maternal feelings of support (MSUP), Maternal conflict with father (MICON), Paternal feelings of affect (PIAF F), Paternal promotion of independence (PIIND), Paternal feelings of support (PISUP), Well-being (WB), Ethnic Identity (ETH) and Social Desirability (SocD). Stepwise multiple regression was used to establish the cause-and- effect relationships (refer to Figure E1, p. 157). All path coefficients were significant at or below the .05 level. Utilizing calculations from Table E (p. 179), the direct, indirect and total causal effects of the model are presented in Table 20 (p. 93). R2 is noted for each endogenous variable. Once again, the outcome of primary interest was adolescent well-being; the determinant with the largest total causal effect was maternal feelings of affect (.604). The remaining determinants of adolescent well-being as indicated by the total causal effect were ethnic identity (.482), paternal feelings of support (.481 ), social desirability (-.373), paternal promotion of independence (.293), maternal promotion of independence (.164), paternal feelings of affect (-.146), and maternal conflict with father (-.003). Note the negative impact of paternal feelings of affect and social desirability. 89 Table 18 Pearson Correlation Coefficients African American and Hispanic Group Endogenous and Exogenous Variables Used in Path 2 MIAFF MIIND MISUP PIAFF PIIND PISUP WB ETH SocD MIAFF 1 .00 MIIND .598" 1.00 MISUP .843“ .304” 1 .00 PIAFF 250* .271* .142 1.00 PIIND .425" .404” .231” .542” 1 .00 PISUP . 108 .299" .032 .878“ .381" 1 .00 WB .672“ .506“ .476“ .508" .61 2“ .378“ 1 .00 ETH .1 34 .023 -.057 .330" 256* .216* .488“ 1 .00 $000 -.494** -.223* -.394** -.233* -.243* .022 -. 543" -.053 1 .00 MICON -.088 -.172 -.235* -.293** -.004 -.255* -.014 .147 243* Maternal promotion of independence (MIIND), Maternal feelings of affect (MIAFF), Maternal feelings of support (MSUP), Paternal feelings of affect (P/AF F), Paternal promotion of independence (PIIND), Paternal feelings of support (PISUP), Well-being (WB), Ethnic Identity (ETH), Social Desirability (SocD) ** Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed) * Conelation is significant at the 0.05 level (2 tailed) Table 19 African American and Hispanic Group Path 2 Observed and Reproduced Correlations MAE F MIIND M/(QN PIIND P/S_UP P/A_FF W§ ETH Observed Correlations MIAFF 1.00 MIIND .598 1.00 MICON -.088 -.172 1.00 Pll ND .425 .404 —.004 1 .00 PISUP .108 .299 -.255 .381 1.00 PIAFF .250 .271 -.293 .542 .108 1.00 WB .672 .506 -.014 .612 .378 .508 1.00 ETH .134 .023 .147 .256 .216 .330 .488 1.00 SocD -.494 -.223 .243 -.243 .022 -.233 -.543 -.053 Reproduced Correlations MIAFF 1.00 MIIND .598 1.00 MICON — — 1.00 PIIND .395 .417 .030 1.00 PISUP .150 .271 -.213 - 1.00 P/AF F .124* .209 -.311 - - 1.00 WB .683 .525 -.003 .539 .457 .117* 1.00 ETH .051 .085 .140 .217 .247 -.325 .374* 1.00 $99 -.476 -.2_§§i .201 -.187 439 -.12_2: -.6_04 1&4 *Difference >08 90 .598” h0¥dqi Figure 4 Input Path Diagram 4: African American/ Hispanic Group Path Analysis 2 I 91 \Ndh bemg .481” This model explained approximately 79% of variance in adolescent well- being. The primary determinant of paternal feelings of affect was maternal conflict with father (—.31 1) with maternal promotion of independence (.209) following. Approximately 27% of the variance in paternal feelings of affect was explained by the model. The primary determinant of paternal feelings of support was maternal promotion of independence (.271) followed by ethnic identity (.247) and maternal conflict with father (-.213). Approximately 19% of the variance in paternal feelings of support was explained by the model. The primary determinant of paternal promotion of independence was maternal feelings of affect (.272), followed by maternal promotion of independence (.244), ethnic identity (.217), and maternal conflict with father (.030). Approximately 26% of variance in paternal promotion of independence was explained by the model. The primary determinant of ethnic identity was paternal feelings of affect (.408), followed by maternal conflict with father (.140) and maternal promotion of independence (.085). Approximately 17% of the variance in ethnic identity was explained by the model. The primary determinant of social desirability was maternal feelings of affect (.476), followed by maternal conflict with father (.201). Approximately 28% of the variance in social desirability was explained by the model. 92 Table 20 Summary for Causal Effects for African American and Hispanic Group Path 2 Outcome Determinant Direct Indirect Total PIAFF MIIND 209* .— .209 (r?2 = .270) MICON -.311* —— -.311 MIAFF —— —- -- * PISUP ETH 247* —— .247 (R2 = .191) MIIND 250* .021 .271+ MICON -.248* .035 -213 MIAFF —- —- —— * PIIND ETH 217* -— .217 (R2 + .262) MIIND 244* -- .244 * MICON -— .030 .030 MIAFF 253* .018 .272+ wa_ MIAFF .354** .250 .604 * (R2 = .792) MIIND -.- .164 .164 * MICON ——- -.003 -.003 PIIND .295** -- .295 PISUP .481** -—- .481 PIAFF -.369* .223 -.146 ETH .363** .119 .482 SocD -.373** -— -.373 * SocD MIAFF -.476** -- -.476 (R2 = .282) MIIND —- .— —— * MICON 201* -— .201 ETH MIAFF nu -— —— * (R2 = .174) MIIND -— .085 .085 MICON 267* -.127 .140 PIAFF .408** —— .408 “Direct effect is significant at he .01 level. *Direct effect is significant at the .05 level. * Total effect my be incomplete due to unanalyzed components 93 Discussion Parental Attachment, Interparental Conflict, and Adolescent Well-being Four major findings emerged from this study. The first major finding concerns the impact of maternal attachment style and overt maternal conflict with fathers on paternal attachment style postdivorce. As predicted, maternal attachment style and maternal conflict with father were found to influence the style of paternal attachment within both groups. Contradictory to what was anticipated, maternal attachment style and maternal conflict with father were found to be unrelated. These findings are significant in that father-child attachment style had been shown to be vulnerable to the relationship children have with their mothers (Doyle, Markiewicz, Brendgen, Lieberman, & Voss, 2000; Hojat, 1998) and with the cooperative parenting relationship (Hetherington, et al., 1982; McKenry, et al., 1992; lhinger-Tallman, et al, 1993; Whiteside & Becker, 2000). Custodial mothers serve as primary caregivers (US. Census Bureau, 2001) and have more direct influence on children’s everyday activities and psychosocial development. The suggestion that the mother-child relationship may benefit from a lack of cooperative parenting and poor father-child relationships (Whiteside & Becker, 2000) was not supported by this study. The data gathered in this study indicated that the level of maternal conflict with father uniquely predicted the style of paternal attachment. Nevertheless, the style of maternal attachment did not impact the relationship between maternal conflict with father and the style of paternal attachment. These findings indicate that it is important to not just evaluate the relationship between maternal and paternal 94 attachment and adolescent well-being, but to do so within the context of family process variables that may explain the parental divorce-child psychosocial adjustment relationship. Interparental conflict has been identified consistently as a major correlate of behavior problems in children across a wide array of family structures and settings (for reviews Davies & Cummings, 1994; Erel & Burman, 1995). However, if the current model is accurate, it appears that within African American, Hispanic, and Caucasian divorced families, interparental conflict damages the father-child bond and thereby negatively affects adolescent well- being. Among the Caucasian adolescents, perceived maternal conflict had a direct negative affect on adolescent well-being, as well as on paternal attachment. Despite racial/ethnic affiliation, the findings of this study suggest the mother-child bond is resilient to and unaffected by perceived mother-initiated interparental conflict. Secure maternal attachment promoted adolescent psychosocial adjustment, independent of the level of maternal conflict with father. The positive correlation between maternal attachment style and paternal attachment suggests that the influence of family relations on the child cannot fully be understood by studying one isolated relationship. When working with adolescents from divorced families, a key component appears to be the maternal style of attachment and how this style of attachment promotes or impedes the A style of paternal attachment perceived by the adolescent (Davies 8. Cummings, 1994; Rothbaum & Weisz, 1994; Braver, et.al., 1991). Among African American and Hispanic college students, the perception of mothers as fostering autonomy corresponded with melings of availability, understanding, acceptance, care, 95 respect and facilitation of autonomy from fathers. In addition, the data revealed an impact of the affective quality of the mother-adolescent relationship on the perception of fathers as promoting independence. However, the data did not reveal a relationship between the perception of mother as a source of support and the style of paternal attachment. Nor did the data reveal a relationship between the affective quality of the mother-adolescent relationship and perceived paternal feelings of support. This suggests that adolescents respond differently to mothers than they do to fathers, especially in divorced families. Among African American and Hispanic college students, mothers treating their adolescents as competent adults may open the door for adolescents and their fathers to establish affective and emotionally supportive relationships. Likewise, it appears that if mothers provide a secure foundation for the affective needs of the adolescent, the adolescent is more receptive to father’s facilitation of autonomy. The second major finding of this study concerns the relative importance of parental attachment and overt maternal conflict with fathers to adolescent well- being postdivorce. Within the African American and Hispanic group, the prediction that secure attachments to both mother and father would be associated with higher levels of general, spiritual, and psychological well-being was supported when the full-scale measures of attachment were analyzed (total causal effects .693 and .523 respectively). The large effects of adolescent- parent relationships on adolescent well-being were similar to other studies of maternal and paternal attachment (Amato & Booth, 1996; Hojat, 1998; Whiteside 96 & Becker, 2000). Nevertheless, interesting results were produced when the attachment subscales were analyzed instead of the full-scale measures. The perception of mother as a source of support had no effect on well-being or on any of the paternal attachment subscales, despite the moderately strong relationship found between maternal feelings of support and well-being in the correlational analysis (r = .377, p < .01). In addition, while maternal promotion of independence appeared to influence paternal promotion of independence, paternal affective quality of relationship, and fathers as source of support, all three of which influence adolescent well-being, maternal promotion of independence did not directly influence adolescent well-being. Correlational data had suggested a moderately strong relationship between maternal promotion of independence and adolescent well-being (r = .377, p < .01), but it was no longer significant once paternal attachment subscales were entered into the analysis. Conversely, maternal affective quality of relationship had a consistent influence on well-being (b = .354, p < .01; r = .351, p < .01), yet it only impacted the paternal attachment subscale of paternal promotion of independence (b = .244, p < .05). The perception of father both as a source of support and as promoting independence positively affected adolescent well-being (b = .481 and .295 respectively, p < .01). A finding of this study that contradicted previous research findings drawn from primarily Caucasian samples (Hetherington, Cox, Cox, 1982; Summers, Forehand, Arrnistead, & Tannenbaum, 1998) was that perceived paternal feelings of affect had a negative direct impact on adolescent well-being (b = -.369, p< .05). 97 These findings are indicative of how adolescents are influenced by their perceptions of their relationships with their mothers and with their fathers postdivorce. Noncustodial fathers have limited direct involvement in the daily lives of their children, Gecas and Schwalbe (1986) suggested that when fathers are involved, the importance of this involvement might be exaggerated in the mind of adolescents and thus take on greater importance than the daily involvement of the mother. Thus, African American and Hispanic college- attending adolescents might perceive mother's facilitation of autonomy and conveyance of support as less important than similar behaviors by fathers. Paternal facilitation of autonomy and conveyance of support may be perceived as indicative of a caring and secure relationship, thereby enhancing the father- child relationship and adolescent well-being. In regards to the affective quality of the relationship within the African American and Hispanic group, the data indicated that perceptions of mother as understanding, accepting, and considerate had a strong positive impact on adolescent well-being, whereas similar perceptions of father had a moderately strong negative impact on adolescent well-being. In secure relationships, parental figures often serve as secure bases from which adolescents explore and as sources of comfort in times of stress (Kems, Klepac, 8. Cole, 1996; Larson, Richards, Moneta, Holmbeck, 8. Duckett, 1996). However, within divorced families, where mothers have physical custody, the limited time with father may consequently make the affective quality of the adolescent-father relationship more labile and vulnerable, thus exerting a paradoxical impact on the 98 adolescent’s sense of well-being in conjunction with attending college. Several studies indicate a precipitous decline in father contact after divorce, with 23% to 30% of sampled children and sampled fathers reporting no contact with each other during the first year postdivorce (F urstenberg, Morgan, & Allison, 1987; Mitchell, 1985; Seltzer, Schaeffer, & Chamg, 1989). Feelings or fear of abandonment of the noncustodial fathers when the student transitions to college may result in the secure father-adolescent affective relationship having a negative impact on well-being (b = -.369). Conversely, in regard to the mother- adolescent relationship that is based on daily contact, the secure mother- adolescent affective relationship may facilitate the transition to college by providing emotional assurance and stability (total effect = .604). Among the Caucasian group, the absence of a relationship between paternal attachment and adolescent well-being in the first path analysis indicates the importance of including the different components of parental attachment in any analysis of parental attachment style. Findings and conclusions based upon the path analysis using the full-scale attachment measures would have been much different than those that were drawn from the analysis using the attachment subscales. These findings indicate that parent—child attachment styles may be much more complicated than can be accurately represented with a single measure. In addition, the correlations between maternal attachment style and paternal attachment suggest that the influence of family relations on the adolescent cannot fully be understood by studying one isolated relationship. 99 In regard to the first major finding of this study (i.e., the impact of maternal attachment style and overt maternal conflict with fathers on paternal attachment style) among Caucasian adolescents from divorced families, maternal promotion of autonomy facilitated the father-adolescent relationship as a source of support. A secure emotive relationship with mother facilitated a secure emotive relationship with father, as well as greatly impacting the adolescent’s sense of security in father’s promotion of autonomy. Maternal conflict with father uniquely predicted the style of paternal attachment within the three attachment subscales. Surprisingly, the perception of the mother-adolescent relationship as a source of support significantly impeded a sense of security and comfort with the father's promotion of autonomy. Thus, the hypothesis that low levels of maternal conflict with father would correspond with secure attachment to father is supported. Maternal attachment did not moderate the relationship between the degree of maternal conflict toward father and the style of paternal attachment. Similar to the African American and Hispanic group, maternal attachment and maternal conflict toward father were not correlated. The hypothesis that secure attachment to mother would correlate with secure attachment with father did not hold true with the attachment subscales of mother as source of support and paternal promotion of independence. The interplay between the promotion of autonomy and source of support subscales is interesting. Whereas secure maternal promotion of autonomy positively correlated with secure paternal feelings of support, secure maternal feelings of support negatively correlated with paternal promotion of autonomy. 100 Within this college-age population, these findings suggest that mothers’ responses to their child’s transitions from childhood to adulthood may significantly impact not only their child, but also, their child’s relationship with their fathers. When the mother-adolescent relationship is secure in the promotion of autonomy, the adolescent experiences his/her father as more emotionally supportive. However, the more adolescents perceive their mothers as sources of emotionally support, the less secure they appear to be with their fathers’ promotion of autonomy. Maternal affective quality had a stronger impact on secure paternal promotion of autonomy than did maternal feelings of support, however, neither paternal promotion of autonomy nor maternal affective quality significantly influenced adolescent well-being. These findings provide some preliminary evidence suggesting that there may be distinct differences between how adolescents from divorced families are influenced by parental attachment styles postdivorce and their subsequent well-being. In regards to the second major finding regarding parental attachment and adolescent well-being, within the Caucasian group, the prediction that secure attachments to both mother and father would be associated with higher levels of general, spiritual, and psychological well-being was not supported when the full- scale measures of attachment were analyzed. Only secure maternal attachment was significantly correlated with higher levels of general, spiritual, and psychological well-being. This finding was not consistent with previous studies that found large effects of adolescent-mother and adolescent-father relationships on adolescent well-being (Amato 8: Booth, 1996; Hojat, 1998; Whiteside & 101 Becker, 2000). When the parental attachment subscales were used in the analysis, adolescent well-being was enhanced by the perception of mothers as sources of emotional support, positive affective qualities of paternal relationships, and low levels of maternal conflict with father. These results support the hypothesis regarding parental attachment and conflict, but provide greater specificity than was theorized. The third major finding of this study concerns the importance of ethnic identity and a response style of social desirability to parental attachment and adolescent well-being. The tendency to respond to self-report measures with a style of social desirability is well documented in the research (Reynolds, 1982) and was found to have a significant impact on the variables in this study. While this response style is found and controlled in numerous research studies, few researchers attempt to explain the relationship between a social desirability response style and their main variables. In this study, a response style of social desirability was found within both the African American and Hispanic group and the Caucasian group and was entered into the path analysis first in order to control for unwanted variance. The differences found between the groups in relation to social desirability were interesting. Within the Caucasian group, social desirability had a moderately strong impact on paternal feelings of affect and paternal feelings of support, but did not correlate with any of the maternal attachment subscales or impact adolescent well-being. Within the African American and Hispanic group, social desirability was impacted by maternal feelings of affect, maternal promotion of independence, and maternal conflict with 102 father. Social desirability also had a moderately strong negative impact on adolescent well-being. These findings provide some preliminary evidence that suggest the tendency to respond in a socially desirable manner is associated with the parent-adolescent relationship, which may be indicative of adolescent well-being, and may differ among racial/ethnic groups. The results of the current study are consistent with Searle’s (1998) finding that social desirability had a differential effect within attachment categories, but contradicted Seiffge’s (2003) finding that securely attached persons reported less mental and physical symptoms but higher social desirability. The relationship between parental-child relationships and the willingness to present oneself in an unfavorable light warrants clarification through further research. Ethnic identity was also found to co-vary with paternal attachment styles within the African American and Hispanic group and was thus included in the path analysis. The data indicated that maternal conflict toward father and perceived paternal feelings of affect positively influenced ethnic identity, while ethnic identity in turn positively influenced paternal feelings of support, paternal promotion of independence, and adolescent well-being. The moderately strong effects between ethnic identity and paternal feelings of affect and adolescent well-being suggest that ethnic identity might play a substantial role in mother- child, father-child, and mother-father relationships, as well as significantly impacting adolescent well-being. These findings suggest that ethnic identity should be considered whenever studying family process variables within racially/ethnically diverse samples. 103 The fourth major finding of this study concerns the differences in parental attachment and adolescent well-being found between the African American and Hispanic group and the Caucasian group. The findings of the current study did not support the hypothesis that no significant differences would be found differentiating Hispanic and African American adolescents’ from Caucasian adolescents’ parental attachment patterns. Several differences in the data have been illustrated thus far, but a few of the more significant include: 1) within the African American and Hispanic group, MIAFF, MIIND, PIIND, PISUP, and P/AF F causally effect adolescent well-being, whereas only MISUP and PIAF F causally effect adolescent well-being within the Caucasian group; 2) maternal conflict with father had a significant negative relation on adolescent well-being within the Caucasian group, but not within the African American and Hispanic group; 3) MIIND causally effect all three paternal attachment subscales within the African American and Hispanic group, but only effects PISUP within the Caucasian group; 4) MISUP had no relationship with PIAFF, PIIND, PISUP, or WB within the African American and Hispanic group, yet MISUP had a moderately strong negative impact on PIIND and a moderately strong positive impact on WB within the Caucasian group; 5) P/AF F had a negative impact on WB within the African American and Hispanic group, but a positive impact on WB within the Caucasian group; 6) ethnic identity was a significant variable within the African American and Hispanic group, but not within the Caucasian group; and 7) the model accounted for 79% of the variance within the African American and Hispanic group, but only 35% of the variance within the Caucasian group. These findings 104 suggest it would be ineffectual and unethical to attend to racially/ethnically diverse family systems and adolescents in the same manner as majority group family systems. The findings from this study were consistent with prior research that found father-adolescent and father-young adult relationships to be significant predictors of young adult psychosocial well-being (Summers, Forehand, Arrnistead, & Tannerbaum, 1998; Whiteside & Becker, 2000). As one of the first studies to examine this relationship within the context of postdivorce parental attachment among African American, Hispanic, and Caucasian college students, this study extended the findings of other researchers who have examined the unique links between 1) divorce and adolescent well-being, 2) parental attachment and adolescent well-being, 3) parental conflict and adolescent well-being and 4) maternal and paternal attachment styles. Clearly, the results of this study do suggest that 1) mother-adolescent bonds and mother-father conflict have an impact on the father-adolescent bonds and 2) mother-adolescent and father- adolescent bonds play an important role in the development of adolescent self- esteem. Clearly, differences exist in the postdivorce family process variables and parental relationships between the racial/ethnic groups. Efforts should be made to replicate this study to establish confidence in these findings and to clarify the robustness of these initial interpretations Limitations There are several limitations inherent in this study. To start with, combining the African American group and Hispanic group due to sample size 105 may limit the generalizability of the results. Future studies should include a more diverse sample that includes a larger number of participants of different races/ethnicities. Secondly, it is important to note that in the process of using path analysis, causal inferences were drawn from correlational data. The degree of confidence in the validity of causal inference from correlational data is typically much weaker than inference drawn from data resulting from a well-designed experimental study where the important concept of random assignment to treatments has been incorporated (Tate, 1998). In path analysis with correlational data there is no statistical test that will definitively indicate whether the specifications, or misspecifications, represent true causal effects. In addition, research such as this that relies entirely on self-report measures of key constructs, and retrospective measure of variables, may be vulnerable to memory and\or distortions. While attention to diversity is apparent in the design, the study relied on a convenient sample from a large mid-westem university identified largely as White. Therefore, appropriate caution should be exercised in generalizing the findings. Efforts were made to control recognized covariates, but there may be confounds such as personality characteristics or other relationship-specific factors. Lastly, the developmental stage of the subjects in this study may have influenced the results. Psychoanalyst Erik Erikson has described the physical, emotional and psychological stages of development and related specific issues, or developmental work or tasks, to each stage. According to Erikson, the subjects in this study were at the stage of development that he called Identity vs 106 Role sat: adc inte [I Role Confusion. Individuals at this stage of life are learning how to answer satisfactorily the question of "Who am I?” but even the most adjusted of adolescents experience some role identity confusion. Attempts are made to integrate many roles (e.g., child, sibling, student, athlete, worker) into a self- image under role models and peer pressure. In later adolescence, the adolescent seeks leadership, and gradually develops a set of ideals. Erikson believes that, in our culture, adolescence do not yet have to "play for keeps,” but can experiment, trying various roles, and thus hopefully find the one most suitable for them. This means that individuals at this age are negotiating their roles with their parents, their beliefs about life, and their ideas about who they are. The impact of this process could have directly or indirectly influenced the results in this study. According to Lewinsohn and Rosenbaum (1987), current emotional states may affect perceptions of the parent-child relationship. In this study, there was a statistically significant difference in the educational level of the two groups (p < .01). Nearly 50% of the African American and Hispanic group were freshman and 19% sophomores, whereas the Caucasian group was dispersed fairly evenly across the educational levels, with 25% within each of the four years. This study should be repeated with attention given to the educational level/age of the participants. Relevance to Practice Findings from this study serve to highlight the changing and diverse nature of parent-child relationships after divorce, as well as the complexity of family dynamics among African American, Hispanic, and Caucasian families. Appraisal 107 of the mother-adolescent, father-adolescent, and mother-father relationships serve as important predictors of multiple areas of adjustment for adolescents and young adults. Adolescent attachment to parents seems to have important implications for adolescent subjective well-being in a broad range of subscales. A significant implication of this study resides in the difference found between the African American and Hispanic group when compared to the Caucasian group. These distinctions indicate that the same modality of treatment cannot be used across racial and ethnic groups. We are once again reminded that, “one size does not fit all.” Therapy needs to be sensitive to the physical and mental features of the divorce process, including loyalty dilemmas, loss, renewal, understanding of clients’ divorce-related experiences, and the reengagement of adult children with their fathers within the cultural context of the individuals being served. Racially/ethnically sensitive interventions need to be identified to help parents handle conflict constructively, support and facilitate cooperative co- parenting, and promote positive child attachment relationships with both parents. Clinicians working with adolescents from divorced families with mother-custody arrangements may want to select intervention strategies that help these adolescents examine and understand their emotional relationships with their fathers, perhaps aiding them in relinquishing a sense of responsibility for conflict while helping them to focus on their ability to take responsibility for the positive happenings in their lives. Findings of this study serve to emphasize specific avenues for interventions to enhance the mother-adolescent relationship and the 108 father-adolescent relationship in distressed divorced families within different racial/ethnic groups. This study suggests that when working with Caucasian college students from divorced families there may be great benefit in assessing paternal relationships and parental conflict, past and present, and subsequent impact on students’ well-being. If emotional support is missing from the student’s relationship with his/her mother, the therapeutic relationship could serve as a model. This relationship and the establishment of a support system will help the student feel understood, build confidence and supply empowerment. Compromised affective quality of the paternal relationship may manifest in a need to control, fear of closeness, and a lack of reciprocity in interpersonal relationships. The therapeutic challenge is to enhance the establishment of secure attachment patterns of trust, affection, intimacy, communication, and reciprocity. Students are assisted in learning to identify, manage, and express emotions in a constructive manner. Alternatively, this study suggests that when working with African American or Hispanic students, compromised affective quality of maternal relationships may result in feelings of isolation, alienation, and disconnect. Treatment may be enhanced by focusing on establishing a sense of connection and belonging, as well as the formulation of identity. Students need to be assisted in exploring the environment with feelings of safety and security, which will then lead to healthy cognitive and social development. A lack of paternal facilitation of autonomy or affective support may challenge the basic qualities of trust, empathy, 109 cooperation, and reciprocity within relationships. The counselor can assist in the modification of negative relationship dynamics, which will enhance stability and support from inside and outside the family, and create a climate of hope, positivity, and closeness. Core beliefs about the maternal relationship are focused on in order to improve the paternal relationship and enable students to create and maintain emotionally reciprocal relationships and cultivate a positive and realistic sense of self and self-in-relation to the world. Additionally, given 1) the increased concern for budgetary restrictions in institutions across the country, 2) the percentage of students actually graduating from college is declining (US. Department of Education, 1995), and 3) once at college, students are reporting record high levels of emotional and psychological stress (Sax, Astin, Korn, & Mahoney, 1999), the option of intervening with students experiencing difficulties via psychoeducational groups focusing on facilitating healthy attachment might be feasible and cost-efficient. Regardless of the mode of intervention, it is important that clinicians working with college students better understand the reasons why some students make it through college successfully while other students become increasing emotionally distressed, resulting in some students experiencing decreased school performance, misconduct, depression, and difficulties with interpersonal relationships. This research offers contributions in gaining understanding in this area, especially in meeting the needs of African American and Hispanic students. 110 Future Research Efforts should be made to replicate this study to establish confidence in these findings and to clarify the robustness of these initial interpretations. Postdivorce family process variables within different racial and ethnic groups need further examination. Efforts should be made to use measures normed on diverse samples or to norm the current measures on African American and Hispanic samples. Future research may find it beneficial to explore the varying levels of noncustodial father contact, the different possible dynamics of the in- residence family composition on the out-of-residence relationship with father, and whether similar findings exist between custodial fathers and noncustodial mothers within and between different racial/ethnic groups. Future studies, perhaps with longitudinal designs, could also yield support for the causal inferences suggested in this study by further delineation of the unique effects of maternal and paternal attachment styles on additional dimensions of adolescent functioning postdivorce. Exploration of the role of ethnic identity in the parent- child and mother-father relationships in the realm of attachment theory could prove to be enlightening. 111 APPENDICES 112 APPENDIX A Forms and Demographic lnforrnation UCHRIS Approval Recruitment Announcement Consent Form Table A1 Caucasian Group Descriptive Statistics for Demographic Information Table A2 Caucasian Group Descriptive Statistics for Subscales and F ull-scale Parental Attachment Measures. Table A3 African American and Hispanic Group Descriptive Statistics for Demographic lnfonnation Table A4 African American and Hispanic Group Descriptive Statistics for Subscales and Full-scale Parental Attachment Measures. Table A5 Cronbach’s Alpha 113 114 114 115 117 117 118 119 119 UCHRIS Approval IRB #04-208 / APP # i019504 Approved 3I20l2004. Renewal approved 2l21l05. Recruitment Script Good day, my name is Janet Kinney and I am a doctoral candidate in Counseling Psychology. I am here today to recruit volunteers to participate in my dissertation research project. Your (instructor) has allowed me to speak with you today about the study and offer an opportunity for those who are interested in participating to complete a survey. The purpose of this study is to learn more about the factors that affect individuals from divorced families. Therefore, only undergraduates that experienced the divorce of their biological parents in childhood are eligible to participate. If you agree to participate in this study, you will be asked to complete paper and pencil surveys that are designed to measure your perceptions of your parents’ attitudes towards you, your relationships with your parents, and your well being. Total time of participation is approximately 30-40 minutes. Participants will have the opportunity to enter a drawing in which they will be eligible to win one of three $100 awards. Those wishing to enter the drawing will need to supply an email address on the consent form, along with their name. Since this study is focusing on factors that affect students from divorced families, there are some specific criteria that apply to participants. Potential subjects must be: a) 18-23 years old, b) single with no children, c) parents divorced prior to age 16, d) mother had primary custody, e) postdivorce contact with both parents, f) 8”1 grade reading level. Your responses to the survey questions will be kept confidential. Furthermore, your name will not appear on any of the measures, and no one will have any way of associating your name with any of the measures. If you volunteer to participate, you are under no obligation to complete the questionnaire packet. In a moment I will be passing out the informed consent forms. If you would like to participate and you meet the study criteria, please take one. If you do not qualify or would just rather not participate, please do not take one. After we go over the informed consent form, I will answer questions and then distribute the survey packet. I thank you for your time. Are there any questions right now? 114 INFORMED CONSENT FORM Postdivorce Parental Relationships and Well-being Among African American, Hispanic, and Caucasian College Students Procedures If you agree to participate in this study, you will be asked to complete paper and pencil instruments that are designed to measure your perceptions of your parents’ attitudes towards you, your relationships with your parents, and your well being. Total time of participation is approximately 30-40 minutes. All participants will have the opportunity to enter a drawing in which they will be eligible to win one of three $100 awards. Those wishing to enter the drawing need to supply an email address on this consent form, along with their name. Consent forms will be randomly selected to determine the winners. Once all data is collected, winners will be asked via email and to supply an address where a check can be mailed. Potential subjects must be: a) 18-23 years old, b) single with no children, c) parents divorced prior to age 16, d mother had primary custody, e) postdivorce contact with both parents, f) 8 grade reading level. Risks/Discomforts There are minimal risks involved in participating in the study. You might find that publicly stating you are from a divorced family is uncomfortable or you may find there are instrument questions that make you uncomfortable or upset. There are several ways to deal with uncomfortable feelings that may arise: a. Participation is strictly voluntary, so there is no pressure to participate. b. You may consider speaking with a counselor about your reactions (e.g., you could contact the MSU Counseling Center at 355-8270). c. You are free to decline to answer any questions you do not wish to answer or to stop your participation at any time. Privacy and Confidentiality Your privacy will be protected to the maximum extent permissible under the law. The following precautions will be taken to protect your confidentiality: a) no individual names or other identifying information will be used in any reports or publications that may result from this study, b) your name will not be on any of the surveys, and 0) both the informed consent forms as well as the completed instruments will be kept in a locked cabinet and only the primary researcher will have access to these files. Participation Your participation in this study is voluntary; you may decline to participate without penalty. If you decide to participate, you may withdraw from the study at anytime without penalty and without loss of benefits to which you are otherwise entitled. 115 Contact If you have questions at any time about the study or the procedures, you may contact the principle investigator, Janet M. Kinney, MA, LLP, at [mkimev3@aol. com or at 51 7-62 7-5490. If you have questions or concerns regarding your rights as a study participant, or are dissatisfied at any time with any aspect of this study, you may contact — anonymously, if you wish - Dr. Peter Vasilenko, Chair of the University Committee on Research Involving Human Subjects (UCRIHS) by phone: (51 7) 355—21 80, fax: (51 7) 432-4503, e-mail: ucn'h§_@msu.edu, or regular mail: 202 Olds Hall, East Lansing, MI 48824. ” Consent I voluntarily agree to participate in this study. Please give the signed form to the interviewer and keep the other copy of the consent form for your records. Participant’s name (print) Participant's signature Email address Date 116 Table A1 Caucasian Group Descriptive Statistics for Demographic Information Std. N Min Max Mean Deviation Age 93 18.00 24.00 19.9677 1.30594 Education Level 93 1.00 5.00 2.4624 1.17547 Age at time of separation 68 1.00 16.00 8.1029 4.82264 Age at time of divorce 93 .00 16.00 7.9247 5.00920 Length of parents committed 93 2.00 26.00 11.6989 5.47878 relationship to each other in years Number of siblings with same 93 .00 4.00 1.1183 .84506 parents Frequency of contact with mother 93 1.00 4.00 1.5484 .71500 currently Frequency of contact with mother 93 1.00 5.00 1.0860 .58339 prior to age 18 Frequency of contact with father 93 1.00 4.00 2.2903 .89176 currently Frequency of contact with father 93 1.00 4.00 2.0430 .87121 prior to age 18 mother's educational level 93 1.00 5.00 2.9785 1.04235 father‘s educational I ev el 93 1.00 5.00 3.1828 1.12236 Table A2 Caucasian Group Descriptive Statistics for Subscales and Full-scale Parental Attachment Measures. I N Min Max Mean Std. Dev. Maternal Attachmennota, 1 93 95.00 245.00 205.0753 33.38420 Maternal feelings of affect 93 37.00 119.00 99.4516 18.28489 Mt I ti 1 ,n§,$:d§;22’° °" ° 93 37.00 80.00 65.5806 10.69978 Mother Wings of sumo“ 93 23.00 70.00 56.4516 11.67636 Paternal Attachment Total 93 90.00 246.00 184.3656 40.87435 Pat al ff ° 1' r @3821?“ “any" 93 38.00 119.00 87.9677 23.65488 Pat I tio r ,,,;,,’2:J;fi’;2° " ° 93 36.00 81.00 64.5699 10.31820 F th r r rt 3 “teams" sum” 93 18.00 68.00 46.1398 14.80716 117 Table A3 African American and Hispanic Group Descriptive Statistics for Demographic lnforrnation N Min Max Mean Std. Dev. Age 86 18.00 23.00 19.6512 1.31746 Education Level 86 1.00 5.00 1.9535 1.06171 Age at time of separation 54 1.00 16.00 6.2778 3.84814 Age at time of divorce 86 1.00 16.00 6.4651 3.75013 Length of parents committed 82 2.00 28.00 9.3415 5.03621 relationship to each other in years Number of siblings with same 85 .00 4.00 1.2941 .99790 parents Frequency of contact with mother 86 1.00 4.00 1.5581 .67918 currently Frequency of contact with mother 86 1.00 3.00 1.1163 .41780 prior to age 18 Frequency of contact with father 86 1.00 5.00 2.7907 .99548 currently Frequency of contact with father 1.00 4.00 2.6279 1.04089 prior to age 18 M oth er’s educational level 86 1.00 5.00 2.9419 .89908 86 1.00 5.00 2.7558 1.20719 Father’s educational level 118 Table A4 African American and Hispanic Group Descriptive Statistics for Subscales and Full-scale Parental Attachment Measures. N Min Max Mean Std. Dev. Maternal Attachment Tot 86 123.00 255.00 196.1744 31.21168 Maternal feelings of 86 55.00 118.00 95.2093 16.04194 affect Maternal promotion of 86 40.00 87.00 63.3721 11.65287 independence Mother feelings of 86 25.00 70.00 52.8605 12.01290 support Paternal Attachment Tot 86 78.00 229.00 169.1395 39.04813 Paternal affective quality 86 40.00 1 15.00 80.2326 20.80423 of relationship Paternal promotion of 86 22.00 78.00 61.0116 14.15003 independence Father feelings of 86 18.00 66.00 41.1744 13.91334 support Table A5 Cronbach’s Alpha Cronbach’s Alpha N of Items State Self-Esteem Scale .896 20 Life Attitude Profile .941 16 Marlowe-Crowne Social Desirability .844 1 3 Scale Multigroup Ethnic Identity Measure .901 12 Perceived Wellness Scale .928 36 Schwarz Inter-Parental Conflict Scale .928 34 Maternal Attachment Scale .948 52 Paternal Attachment Scale .957 52 119 APPENDIX B African American and Hispanic Group Path Analysis 1 Figure B1 African American and Hispanic Group Stepwise Multiple 121 Regression for Path Analysis 1 Figure B2 African American and Hispanic group Normal Probability Plot 131 of the Regression Standardized Residuals for Path Analysis 1 Figure B3 African American and Hispanic Residuals Scatter plot for Path 132 Analysis 1 Table B African American and Hispanic Group Path 1 Decompositions 133 and Calculations of Reproduced Correlations 120 Figure 81 African American and Hispanic Group Stepwise Multiple Regression for Path Analysis 1 B.1.1 Step 1: Well-being regressed on ethnic identity and social desirability, then maternal attachment, maternal conflict with father, and paternal attachment. Model Summary Adjusted Std. Error of Model R R Square R Square the Estimate 1 .644a .415 .408 2.06080 2 .788b .621 .612 1.66894 3 835° .697 .686 1 .50009 4 .870d .756 .744 1.35385 5 883° .780 .767 1 .29391 a. Predictors: (Constant), Maternal Attachment Total b. predictors: (Constant), Maternal Attachment Total, eth_ mean 0- Predictors: (Constant), Maternal Attachment Total, eth_ mean, Social Desirability Scale Mean d. Predictors: (Constant), Maternal Attachment Total, eth_ mean, Social Desirability Scale Mean, Paternal Attachment Total 9- Predictors: (Constant), Maternal Attachment Total, eth_ mean, Social Desirability Scale Mean, Paternal Attachment Total, Schwarz inter-parental conflict scale total 121 Allow! Sum of Model Squares df Mean Square F Sig. _1 R' egression 252.979 1 252.979 59.568 .0003 Residual 356.740 84 4.247 Total 609.719 85 2 Regression 378.535 2 189.267 67.951 .000” Residual 231.184 83 2.785 Total 609.719 85 3 Regression 425.196 3 141.732 62.984 000° Residual 184.523 82 2.250 Total 609.719 85 4 Regression 461.252 4 115.313 62.912 .0006 Residual 148.467 81 1.833 Total 609.719 85 5 Regression 475.782 5 95.156 56.836 .0008 Residual 133.937 80 1.674 Total 609.719 85 a. Predictors: (Constant), Maternal Attachment Total b- Predictors: (Constant), Matamal Attachment Total, eth_mean C- Predictors: (Constant), Maternal Attachment Total, eth_mean, Social Desirability Scale Mean d- Predictors: (Constant), Maternal Attachment Total, eth_mean, Social Desirability Scale Mean, Paternal Attachment Total 9- Predictors: (Constant), Maternal Attachment Total, eth_mean, Social Desirability Scale Mean, Paternal Attachment Total, Schwarz inter-parental conflict scale total f. Dependent Variable: sse_total 122 Coetflclenti Unstandardized Standardized Coefficients Coefficients Model B Std. Error Beta t . Moment) 2.247 1.422 1.580 .118 Maternal Attachment Totq .055 .007 .644 7.718 .000 2 (Constant) -5.696 1.651 3.449 .001 Maternal Attachment To .053 .006 .620 9.153 .000 eth_mean 2.552 .380 .454 6.714 .000 3 (Constant) J -1.000 1.807 -.553 .582 Maternal Attachment Tot .042 .006 .486 7.203 .000 eth_mean 2.501 .342 .445 7.316 .000 3223' Des'rab'w 3w" -3.406 .748 -.307 4554 .000 4 (Constant) -1.282 1.632 -.785 .435 Maternal Attachment Tot .035 .005 .403 6.315 .000 eth_mean 2.027 .327 .361 6.209 .000 3:? Des'rabm 8°“ -3.307 .675 -.298 4.895 .000 Paternal Attachment Tota .019 .004 .274 4.435 .000 5 (Constant) -1 .642 1 .565 -1 .049 .297 Maternal Attachment To .035 .005 .407 6.675 .000 eth_mean 1 .797 .322 .320 5.584 .000 3:2“ “gram" 3““ -3.672 .657 -.331 -5.585 .000 Paternal Attachment Tota .022 .004 .318 5.224 .000 imgwgrggfi‘a' .013 .004 .167 2.946 .004 a. Dependent Variable: sse_total 123 Excluded Variable! Collinearity Parfla' StatIStICS Model 2 Beta In t Sig. Correlation Tolerance 1 Sihwalz inter-parental a conflict scale total .110 1.296 .199 .141 .965 3”“ Des'rab'm 3"“ -.3248 -3.753 .000 -.381 .811 ean eth_mean .4548 6.714 .000 .593 .997 Paternal Attachment Tota .4118' 5.347 .000 .506 .887 2 Schwarz inter-parental b conflict scale total .036 .520 .605 .057 .940 . . .. b 3”” Des'rab'm’ sm'e -.307 .4554 .000 -.449 .810 can Paternal Attachment Totq .284b 4.066 .000 .410 .791 3 Schwarz inter-parental ' c conflict scale total .094 1.487 .141 .163 .906 Paternal Attachment To 274° 4.435 .000 .442 .790 4 Schwarz inter-parental d conflict scale total .167 2.946 .004 .313 .851 3- Predictors in the Model: (Constant), Maternal Attachment Total b- Predictors in the Model: (Constant), Maternal Attachment Total, eth_mean 0- Predictors in the Model: (Constant), Maternal Attachment Total, eth_mean, Social Desirability S Mean d- Predictors in the Model: (Constant), Maternal Attachment Total, eth_mean, Social Desirability S Mean, Paternal Attachment Total e. Dependent Variable: sse_total 3.12 Step 2: Paternal attachment regressed on ethnic identity and social desirability, then maternal attachment and maternal conflict with father. Model Summary Adjusted Std. Error of Model R R Square R Sgare the Estimate 1 .336a .113 .103 36.99038 2 .457b .209 .190 35.15191 3 .507° .257 .230 34.25979 a. Predictors: (Constant), Maternal Attachment Total b. predictors: (Constant), Maternal Attachment Total, eth_ mean 6- Pmdictors: (Constant), Maternal Attachment Total, eth_ mean, Schwarz inter-parental conflict scale total 124 Allowr' Sum of Model Squares df Mean Square F Si . 1 egression 14668.133 1 14668.133 10.720 .0028 Residual 1 14936.2 84 1368.288 Total 1296043 85 2 Regression 27044.816 2 13522.408 10.943 000" Residual 1025595 83 1235.657 Total 1296043 85 3 Regression 33358.196 3 1 1 1 19.399 9.474 .000° Residual 96246.130 82 1 173.733 Total 1296043 85 a. Predictors: (Constant), Maternal Attachment Total b- Predictors: (Constant), Maternal Attachment Total, eth_mean c. Predictors: (Constant), Maternal Attachment Total, eth_mean, Schwarz inter-parental conflict scale total d- Dependent Variable: Paternal Attachment Total Coefficient! Unstandardized Standardized Coefficients Coefficients Model B Std. Error Beta t Si . _1__—(Oonstant) 86.573 25.531 3.391 .001 Maternal Attachment Totq .421 .129 .336 3.274 .002 2 (Constant) 7.710 34.779 .222 .825 Maternal Attachment To .400 .122 .320 3.269 .002 eth_mean 25.340 8.007 .309 3.165 .002 3 (Constant) 24.368 34.649 .703 .484 Maternal Attachment To .344 .122 .275 2.832 .006 eth_mean 28.284 7.906 .345 3.577 .001 mmz';$fiem' -.252 .109 -.228 -2.319 .023 a. Dependent Variable: Patamal Attachment Total 125 Excluded Varlablal Collinearity Partial Statistics ”Odd Beta In t Sig. Correlation Tolerance 1 Schwarz inter-parental a conflict scale total -.171 -1.654 .102 -.179 .965 Social Desirability a Scale Mean -.044 *2384 .702 -,O42 .811 eth_mean .309a 3.165 .002 .328 .997 2 Schwarz inter-parental b conflict scale total -.228 -2.319 .023 -.248 .940 Social Desirability 5 Scale Mean -.033 -.301 .764 -.033 .810 3 Social Desirability c Scale Mean .014 .133 .895 .015 .780 a. Pmdictors in the Model: (Constant), Maternal Attachment Total b- Predictors in the Model: (Constant), Maternal Attachment Total, eth_mean C- Predictors in the Model: (Constant), Maternal Attachment Total, eth_mean, Schwarz inter-parer conflict scale total ‘1 Dependent Variable: Paternal Attachment Total B.1.3 Step 3: Paternal attachment regressed on ethnic identity, then maternal attachment and maternal conflict with father. Model Summary Adjusted Std. Error of Model R R Square R Square the Estimate 1 .336“ .113 .103 36.99038 2 .457b .209 .190 35.15191 3 .507° .257 .230 34.25979 3- Predictors: (Constant), Maternal Attachment Total b. predictors: (Constant), Maternal Attachment Total, eth_ mean c. predictors: (Constant), Matemal Attachment Total, eth_ mean, Schwarz inter-parental conflict scale total 126 ANOVA‘ Sum of Model uares df Mean uare F SQ . T—‘lfegression 148268.133 1 14628133 10.720 .002“ ‘ Residual 1 14936.2 84 1368.288 Total 1296043 85 2 Regression 27044.816 2 13522.408 10.943 .000b Residual 1025595 83 1235.657 Total 1296043 85 3 Regression 33358. 196 3 1 1 1 19.399 9.474 .000° Residual 96246130 82 1 173.733 Total 1296043 85 a. Predictors: (Constant), Maternal Attachment Total b- Predictors: (Constant), Maternal Attachment Total, eth_mean 0- Predictors: (Constant), Maternal Attachment Total, eth_mean, Schwarz inter-parental conflict scale total d- Dependent Variable: Paternal Attachment Total Coefficient! Unstandardized Standardized Coefficients Coefficients Model B Std. Error Beta t Sig. 1 ( slant) 86.573 25.531 3.391 .001 Maternal Attachment Totq .421 .129 .336 3.274 .002 2 (Constant) 7.710 34.779 .222 .825 Maternal Attachment To .400 .122 .320 3.269 .002 eth_mean 25.340 8.007 .309 3.165 .002 3 (Constant) 24.368 34.649 .703 .484 Maternal Attachment T .344 .122 .275 2.832 .006 eth_mean 28.284 7.906 .345 3.577 .001 mwgggem' -252 .109 -.228 -2.319 .023 a. Dependent Variable: Paternal Attachment Total 127 Excluded Varlabla‘ Collinearity Partial Statistics Model Beta In t Sig. Correlation Tolerance I gd'wa'z inter'pmma' 1718 1 654 102 179 965 conflict scale total '° ' ' ° " ° eth_mean .309a 3.165 .002 .328 .997 2 Schwarz inter-parental b conflict scale total -.228 -2.319 .023 -.248 .940 a. Predictors in the Model: (Constant), Maternal Attachment Total 9 Predictors in the Model: (Constant), Maternal Attachment Total, eth_mean C- Dependent Variable: Patemal Attachment Total B.1.4 Step 4: Ethnic identity regressed on maternal attachment, maternal conflict with father and paternal attachment. Model Summary R Square Adjusted R Square Std. Error of l the Estimate [Model I R L1 l .327a .107 .096 .45339 | a. Predictors: (Constant), Paternal Attachment Total ANOVA" Model 1 Sum of Squares (It Mean Square Si Regression Residual Total 2.064 17.267 19.331 1 34 85 2.064 10.041 .206 .002a a. Predictors: (Constant), Paternal Attachment Total P- Dependent Variable: eth_mean Coefficient! Model Unstandardized Coefficients Standardized Coefficients Std. Error Beta t Sig. 1 (Constant) Paternal Attachment Total 2. .004 599 .219 .001 .327 11.893 3.169 .000 .002 a. Dependant Variable: eth_mean 128 Excluded Variable! Model 1 Collinearity Partial Statistics Beta In t Sig. Correlation Tolerance rz inter-parental a conflict scale total .234 2.264 .026 .241 .948 Maternal Attachment Totq -.063° -.573 .568 -.063 .887 3- Predictors in the Model: (Constant), Paternal Attachment Total b- Dependent Variable: eth_mean B.1.5 Step 5: Social desirability regressed on maternal attachment, paternal attachment, and maternal conflict with father. Model Summary [ I Adjusted Std. Error of 1 Model R R Square R Square the Estimate l 1 | .4358 .189 .180 .21892 ] a. Predictors: (Constant), Maternal Attachment Total ANOVAb Sum of Model Squares df Mean Square F Sig. . 1 Regression .940 1 .940 1 9.624 .0003 Residual 4.026 84 .048 Total 4.966 85 a. Predictors: (Constant), Maternal Attachment Total b- Dependent Variable: Social Desirability Scale Mean Coefficients Unstandardized Standardized Coefficients Coefficients Model B Std. Error Beta t Sig. ‘ 1 (Constant) 1.332 .151 8.815 .000 Maternal Attachment Totq -.003 .001 -.435 -4.430 .000 a. Dependent Variable: Social Desirability Scale Mean 129 Excluded Variable! Collinearity Partial Statistics Model Beta In t Sig. Correlation Tolerance _"l51 atemal Attachment To -.040* -.384 .702 -.042 .887 Schwarz inter-parental a conflict scale total .168 1.694 .094 .183 .965 a. Predictors in the Model: (Constant), Maternal Attachment Total b- Dependent Variable: Social Desirability Scale Mean B.1.6 Step 6: Maternal conflict with father regressed on ethnic identity and social desirability first, then maternal attachment and paternal attachment. Model Summary I I Adjusted Std. Error of Model R Square R Square the Estimate | 1 I .2283 .052 .041 34.58256 | a. Predictors: (Constant), Paternal Attachment Total ANOVA" Sum of Model Squares df Mean Square F Sig. 1 1 Regression 551 1.622 1 551 1 .622 4.609 .035‘ Residual 1004601 84 1 195.954 Total 1059717 85 8- Predictors: (Constant), Paternal Attachment Total b- Dependent Variable: Schwarz inter-parental conflict scale total Coefficlents' Unstandardized Standardized Coefficients Coefficients Model 8 Std. Errgr Beta t Sig. onstant) 96.043 16.670 5.761 .000 Paternal Attachment Total -.206 .096 -.228 -2.147 .035 a. Dependent Variable: Schwarz inter-parental conflict scale total 130 Excluded Varlabled’ l Collinearity Partial Statistics Model Beta In t Sig. Correlation Tolerance 1 eth_mean .248.3 2.264 .026 .241 .893 Maternal Attachment Totall -.124‘I -1.100 .275 -.120 .887 8- Predictors in the Model: (Constant), Paternal Attachment Total b- Dependent Variable: Schwarz inter—parental conflict scale total Figure 82 African American and Hispanic group Normal Probability Plot of the Regression Standardized Residuals for Path Analysis 1 Normal P-P Plot of Regression Standardized Residual Dependent Variable: sse_total 1.0 0.8- .o 2 o. E 0.6— :1 0 g. 0.4- ' 0.2- 0.0—, I I l i 0.0 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 Observed Cum Prob 131 Figure 83 African American and Hispanic Residuals Scatter plot for Path Analysis 1 Scatterplot Dependent Variable: sse_total 34 Tu 3 E 2_ O O 1: 0 o 3 .5 _ o o O O E 1 8 ° 0 .3 o O O O O I: o o 0° 0 5 o— 0 O O ° 8 ° " O O 0 g -1"' O o O O m 3’ ° o .2— O V I I I I -3 .2 -1 0 1 Regression Standardized Predicted Value 132 Table B African American and Hispanic Group Path 1 Decompositions and Calculations of Reproduced Correlations Reproduced r’s Path Decompositions r r13 r14 r15 r16 931 (-.435) (D) 9 41 (.336) = .336 (0) 941945 (.336)(.327) (I) 961 "' 941964+ 941954965 "' 931963 (407) + (. 336)(. 318) + (. 336)(. 327)(. 320) + (-. 435)(-. 331)‘ - .693 (D) (I) (I) (I) -.435 .109 r24 = p42 = (--228) = -.228 (D) r25 = 942954 = (-.228) (.327) = --075 ( | ) r 25 = 942964 + 942954965 = (-.228)(.318) + (-.228)(.327)(.320) = -.096 ( l ) ( I ) r34 = 931941 = (-.435)(.336) = '-146 (S) f35 = 931941945 = ( -.435)(.336)(.327) = ._Q43 (3) ’36 = 963 T 931941964 T 931941945965 = (- .331) + (- ..435)( 336)(. 318) + (- 435)(. 336)(. 327)(. 320) = -.393 (D) (S) (S) r45 = 954 = (.327) = .327 (D) ’46 = 964 T 954965 "’ 941931963 "’ 941961 = (.318) + (.327)(.320) + (.336)(-.435)(-.331) + (.336)(.407) = .608 (D) ( l ) (S) (S) ['56 p65 + p54p64 (.320) + (.327) (.318) = .424 (D) (S) 133 APPENDIX C Caucasian Group Path Analysis 1 Figure C1 Caucasian Group Stepwise Multiple Regression for Path Analysis 1 Figure 02 Caucasian group Normal Probability Plot of the Regression Standardized Residuals for Path Analysis 1 Figure C3 Caucasian Group Residuals Scatter plot for Path Analysis 1 134 135 140 141 Figure C1 Caucasian Group Stepwise Multiple Regression for Path Analysis 1 0.1.1 Step 1: Well-being regressed on social desirability, then maternal attachment, maternal conflict toward father and paternal attachment. Model Summary R group = Caucasian Adjusted Std. Error of Model (Selected) R Square R Square the Estimate 1 .3993 .160 .150 1.94619 2 .539” .290 .274 1.79837 8- Predictors: (Constant), Maternal Attachment Total 9- Predictors: (Constant), Maternal Attachment Total, Schwarz inter-parental conflict scale total Allowed Sum of Model Squares df Mean Square F Sig. ‘ 1 egression 65.437 1 65.437 17.276 .0003 Residual 344.675 91 3.788 Total 410.1 12 92 2 Regression 119.040 2 59.520 18.404 , .000b Residual 291.073 90 3.234 Total 410.1 12 92 a. Predictors: (Constant), Maternal Attachment Total b- Predictors: (Constant), Maternal Attachment Total, Schwarz inter-parental conflict scale total C- Dependent Variable: sse_total ‘1 Selecting only cases for which group = Caucasian 135 Coelflclants‘r" Unstandardized Standardized Coefficients Coefficients Model 8 Std. Error Beta t Sig. ‘ 1 (Constant) 8.195 1.263 6.490 .000 Maternal Attachment Tota‘ .025 .006 .399 4.156 .000 2 (Constant) 9.348 1 .201 7.786 .000 Maternal Attachment Tota .024 .006 .385 4.330 .000 Schwarz inter-parental conflict scale total -.020 .005 -.362 -4.071 .000 3- Dependent Variable: sse_total 9- Selecting only cases for which group = Caucasian Excluded Variables Collinearity Partial Statistics Model Beta In t Sit Correlation Tolerance 1 Social sirability Scale 8 Mean -.089 -.922 .359 -.097 .999 Schwarz inter-parental a conflict scale total -.362 4.071 .000 -.394 .998 Paternal Attachment Totq .2728 2.861 .005 .289 .946 2 Social Desirability Scale b Mean -.116 -1.304 .195 -.137 .993 Paternal Attachment To .173b 1.820 .072 .189 .853 3- Predictors in the Model: (Constant), Maternal Attachment Total b- Predictors in the Model: (Constant), Maternal Attachment Total, Schwarz inter-parental conflict : total 9» Dependent Variable: sse__total C.1.2 Step 2: Well-being regressed on maternal attachment and maternal conflict. Model Summary R group = Caucasian Adjusted Std. Error of Model (Selected) R Squ_are R Square the Estimate 1 .3998 .160 .150 1.94619 2 .539b .290 .274 1.79837 a. Predictors: (Constant), Maternal Attachment Total b- Predictors: (Constant), Maternal Amachment Total, Schwarz inter-parental conflict scale total 136 ANOVA“ Sum of Model Squares df Mean Square F Sig. 1 Regression 65.437 1 65.437 17.276 .000“ Residual 344.675 91 3.788 Total 410.112 92 2 Regression 119.040 2 59.520 18.404 .000b Residual 291 .073 90 3.234 Total 410.112 92 a. Predictors: (Constant), Maternal Attachment Total b- Predictors: (Constant), Maternal Attachment Total, Schwarz inter-parental conflict scale total C- Dependent Variable: sse_total d- Selecting only cases for which group = Caucasian Coel'ficient9*b Unstandardized Standardized Coefficients Coefficients Model B Std. Error Beta t Sig. 1 (Constant) 8.195 1 .263 6.490 .000 Maternal Attachment Totq .025 .006 .399 4.156 .000 2 (Constant) 9.348 1.201 7.786 .000 Maternal Attachment To .024 .006 .385 4.330 .000 Schwarz inter-parental ' conflict scale total -.020 .005 -.362 -4.071 .000 a. Dependent Variable: sse_total 9- Selecting only cases for which group = Caucasian Excluded Variables Collinearity Partial Statistics Model Beta In t Sig. Correlation Tolerance 1 chwarz inter-parental a conflict scale total -.362 -4.071 .000 -.394 .998 Paternal Attachment T .272“ 2.861 .005 .289 .946 Ms°°'a' Des'rab'w scab 4089’ -.922 .359 -.097 .999 can 2 Paternal Attachment To .173” 1.820 .072 .189 .853 Social Desirability Scale b Mean -.116 -1.304 .195 -.137 .993 a. Predictors in the Model: (Constant), Maternal Attachment Total b- Predictors in the Model: (Constant), Maternal Attachment Total, Schwarz inter-parental conflict: total 0- Dependent Variable: sse_total 137 0.13 Step 3: Paternal attachment regressed on social desirability, then Model Summary R group = Caucasian Adjusted Std. Error of Model (Selected) R Squ_are R Square the Estimate 1 .314‘:1 .098 .088 39.02581 2 .454b .206 .189 36.81704 3 .510° .260 .235 35.75526 a. Predictors: (Constant), Schwarz inter—parental conflict scale total b- Predictors: (Constant), Schwarz inter-parental conflict scale total, Social Desirability Scale Mean 0- Predictors: (Constant), Schwarz inter-pawntal conflict scale total, Social Desirability Scale Mean, Maternal maternal attachment and maternal conflict with father. Attachment Total ANOVK‘J Sum of ‘ Model Squares df Mean Square F Sig. 1 Regression 15111.341 1 15111.341 9.922 .0023 Residual 1385942 91 1523.014 Total 1 53705.6 92 2 Regression 31711.042 2 15855.521 11.697 .000” Residual 1219945 90 1355.495 Total 1 53705.6 92 3 Regression 39924.51 1 3 13308.170 10.410 .000° Residual 1 1 3781 .1 89 1278.439 Total 1537056 92 a. Predictors: (Constant), Schwarz inter-parental conflict scale total b- Predictors: (Constant), Schwarz inter-parental conflict scale total, Social Desirability Scale Mean 0- Predictors: (Constant), Schwarz inter-parental conflict scale total, Social Desirability Scale Mean, Maternal Attachment Total d- Dependent Variable: Paternal Attachment Total 9- Selecting only cases for which group = Caucasian 138 W Unstandardized Standardized Coefficients Coefficients Model B Std. ErroL Beta t Slg' . 1 (Constant) 200.535 6.537 30.679 .000 Schwarz inter-parental conflict scale total -.333 .106 -.314 -3.150 .002 2 (Constant) 226.981 9.754 23.271 .000 Schwarz inter-parental conflict scale total -.359 .100 -.338 -3.590 .001 18:.“ Des'rabmty scab -31.003 8.859 -.330 3499 .001 an 3 (Constant) 169.017 24.753 6.828 .000 Schwarz inter-parental conflict scale total -.350 .097 -.329 -3.598 .001 “SAW” ms'mbmty scab 31.755 8.609 -.338 -3.689 .000 can Maternal Attachment Total .283 .1 12 .231 2.535 .013 a. Dependent Variable: Paternal Attachment Total b- Selecting only cases for which group = Caucasian Excluded Variables Collinearity Parfia' StatIStICS Model _ Beta In t Sig. Correlation Tolerance 1 Maternal Attachment To .220a 2.256 .026 .231 .998 SM°°'a' Des'rab'm sca'e -330" 3499 .001 -.346 .994 can 2 Maternal Attachment Total .231b 2.535 .013 .259 .997 a. Predictors in the Model: (Constant), Schwarz inter-parental conflict scale total b- Predictors in the Model: (Constant), Schwarz inter-parental conflict scale total, Social Desirabilit Scale Mean 0- Dependent Variable: Paternal Attachment Total C.1.4 Step 4: Maternal conflict regressed on maternal conflict with father and social desirability. No relationship found. 0.15 Step 5: Social desirability regressed on maternal attachment and maternal conflict with father. No relationship found. 139 Figure C2 Caucasian group Normal Probability Plot of the Regression Standardized Residuals for Path Analysis 1 Normal P-P Plot of Regresslon Standardized Residual Dependent Variable: sse_total 1.6 0.8- .o 2 a ,,,,,,, E 0.6-1 :1 ..... 0 g 0.4- 02- 0-0 I l T I r 0.0 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 Observed Cum Prob 140 Figure C3 Caucasian Group Residuals Scatter plot for Path Analysis 1 Regression Standardized Resldual Scatterplot Dependent Variable: sse_total o 0 o o o o % 00 O O o o 0 ° 0 o 8 0,, o O O 00 O o o o oo o o o oo 00 (g o o o o I I l r .2 -1 0 1 Regression Standardized Predicted Value 141 APPENDIX D Caucasian Group Path Analysis 2 Figure D1 Caucasian Group Stepwise Multiple Regression for Path Analysis Figure D2 Caucasian Group Normal Probability Plot of the Regression Standardized Residuals of Path Analysis 2 Figure D3 Caucasian Group Residuals Scatter plot for Path Analysis 2 Table D Caucasian Group Path 2 Decompositions and Calculations of Reproduced Correlations Figure D4 Caucasian Group Path 2 Paternal Promotion of Independence Figure D5 Caucasian Group Path 2 Paternal Feelings of Affect Figure D6 Caucasian Group Path 2 Paternal Feelings of Support 142 143 153 153 154 156 156 156 Figure Dl Caucasian Group Stepwise Multiple Regression for Path Analysis 2 0.1.1 Step 1: Well-being was regressed on social desirability, then on maternal affective quality of relationship, maternal promotion of independence, mother as source of support, maternal conflict with father, paternal feelings of affect, father promotion of independence, and father feelings of support. Modal Summary Adjusted Std. Error of Model R R Square R Square the Estimate 1 .3773 .142 .133 1.96596 2 .553b .306 .290 1 .77892 3 .593° .352 .330 1 .72800 a. Predictors: (Constant), Schwarz inter-parental conflict scale total 9. Predictors: (Constant), Schwarz inter-parental conflict scale total, mother as source of support 0- Predictors: (Constant). Schwarz inter-parental conflict scale total, mother as source of support, paternal affective quality of relationahip Allowr' Sum of Model Squares df Mean Square F Sig. 1 Regression 58.398 1 58.398 1 5.1 10 .000‘I Residual 351 .714 91 3.865 Total 410.1 12 92 2 Regression 125.302 2 62.651 19.798 .000” Residual 284.810 90 3.165 Total 410.1 12 92 3 Regression 144.360 3 48.120 16.1 15 .000° Residual 265.753 89 2.986 Total 410.112 92 a. Predictors: (Constant), Schwarz inter-parental conflict scale total b- Predictors: (Constant), Schwarz inter-parental conflict scale total, mother as source of support C- Predictors: (Constant), Schwarz inter-parental conflict scale total, mother as source of support, paternal affective quality of relationahip d. Dependent Variable: sse_total 143 Coetflclantt Unstandardized Standardized Coefficients Coefficients Model B Std. Error Beta t SE. ‘ I (Co—listant) 14.381 .329 43.674 .000 Schwarz inter-parental conflict scale total -.021 .005 -.377 -3.887 .000 2 (Constant) 10.323 .932 1 1.082 .000 Schwarz inter-parental conflict scale total -.022 .005 -.405 -4.603 .000 mother as source of support .073 .016 .405 4.598 .000 3 (Constant) 8.702 1 .109 7.845 .000 Schwarz inter-parental conflict scale total -.019 .005 -.350 . -3.968 .000 mother as source of support .068 .016 .377 4.374 .000 paternal affective quality of relationahip .020 .008 .224 2.526 .013 a. Dependent Variable: sse_total 144 Excluded Variables Collinearity Partial Statistics Model Beta In t Sig Correlation Tolerance 1 2333:?“ -.102‘3 -1.052 .296 -.110 .994 :uaaiimImghip 343° 3.777 .000 .370 1.000 mdfpaezfifm' .315‘1 3.359 .001 .334 .962 gm” ”um °f 405° 4.598 .000 .436 .995 mfigaéfamgzamp 2738 2.835 .006 .286 .944 might” °f .1568 1.497 .138 .156 .858 323;? ”um °f .242la 2.449 .016 .250 .913 2 3333:?“ -.131b -1495 .138 -.157 .990 rfigymé'ngmp -.080b -.413 .681 -.044 .209 31:23:13?" .128b 1.199 .234 .126 .674 :jg’mfamfimp .224b 2.526 .013 .259 .928 Efilfigflmm °f .077b .798 .427 .084 .828 2:23;? ”um °f .174” 1.888 .062 .196 .335 3 33:33:23“ -.060° -.644 .521 -.068 .856 rufigmslraemxsmp -.191° -1.000 .320 -.106 .199 mfligirfiw 106° 1.015 .313 .108 .669 mgzzgimm °f -205° -1.507 .135 -.159 .386 21%;? ”um °f -.034c -.218 .828 -.023 .305 a. Predictors in the Model: (Constant), Schwarz inter-parental conflict scale total b- Predictors in the Model: (Constant), Schwarz inter-parental conflict scale total, mother as souro of support 9 Predictors in the Model: (Constant), Schwarz inter-parental conflict scale total, mother as sourc of support, paternal affective quality of relationahip d- Dependent Variable: sse_total 145 0.12 Step 2: Paternal feelings of affect was regressed on social desirability, then on maternal feelings of affect, maternal promotion of independence, mother feelings of support, and maternal conflict with father. Model Summary Adjusted Std. Error of Model R R Square R Sware the Estimate 1 .3263 .106 .096 22.48567 2 .419b .175 .157 21.72001 3 466° .217 .191 21.28091 a. Predictors: (Constant), Social Desirability Scale Mean b- Predictors: (Constant), Social Desirability Scale Mean, Schwarz inter-parental conflict scale total C- Predictors: (Constant), Social Desirability Scale Mean, Schwarz inter-parental conflict scale total, Mattemal affective quality of relationship ANOVA" Sum of (m— Squares df Mean Square F Sig.__ 1 Regression 5468.836 1 5468.836 10.816 .001a Residual 46010068 91 505.605 Total 51478.903 92 2 Regression 9020.604 2 4510.302 9.561 .000” Residual 42458.299 90 471 .759 Total 51478.903 92 3 Regression 11172.840 3 3724.280 8.224 000° Residual 40306.064 89 452.877 Total 51478.903 92 a. Predictors: (Constant), Social Desirability Scale Mean b- Predictors: (Constant), Social Desirability Scale Mean, Schwarz inter-parental conflict scale total c. Predictors: (Constant), Social Desirability Scale Mean, Schwarz inter-parental conflict scale total, Mattemal affective quality of relationship d- Dependent Variable: paternal affective quality of relationahip 146 Coefficientf Unstandardized Standardized Coefficients Coefficients Model B Std. Error Beta t Slg' . 1 1 (find 102.382 4.964 20.624 .000 Social Desirability Scale Mean 47.746 5.396 -.326 -3.289 .001 2 (Constant) 1 1 1.109 5.754 19.309 .000 Social Desirability Scale Mean -18.813 5.226 -.346 -3.599 .001 Schwarz inter-parental conflict scale total -.162 .059 -.263 -2.744 .007 3 (Constant) 84.629 13.392 6.320 .000 Social Desirability Scale Mean -18.776 5.121 -.345 -3.667 .000 Schwarz inter-parental conflict scale total -.159 .058 -.259 -2.751 .007 Mattemal affective quality of relationship .265 .121 .205 2.180 .032 a. Dependent Variable: paternal affective quality of relationahip 147 Excluded Variables Collinearity Partial Statistics Model Beta In t SIL Correlation Tolerance 1 Was ”me °f .128’ 1.295 .199 .135 .996 mflifigfm' -.263a 2744 .007 -273 .994 fifimflmfigm .2108 2.165 .033 .222 1.000 {figfl’gnzzfj'm' °f 200" 2.045 .044 .211 .997 2 310%” ”me °f .149b 1.556 .123 .163 .990 :figymgmxmp .205b 2.180 .032 .225 999 {figgnfifc‘j'ta‘m °f .155” 1.598 .114 .167 .960 3 $3338 ”we °f -1696 -.812 .419 -.086 .203 mgnzzfcimaw °f .033c .264 .792 .028 .556 a. Predictors in the Model: (Constant), Social Desirability Scale Mean b- Predictors in the Model: (Constant), Social Desirability Scale Mean, Schwarz inter-parental conflict scale total C~ Predictors in the Model: (Constant), Social Desirability Scale Mean, Schwarz inter-parental conflict scale total, Mattemal affective quality of relationship d- Dependent Variable: paternal affective quality of relationahip D.1.3 Step 3: Paternal promotion of independence was regressed on social desirability, then on maternal feelings of affect, maternal promotion of independence, mother feelings of support, and maternal conflict with father. Modal Summary Adjusted Std. Error of Model R R Square R Square the Estimate 1 .377“ .142 .133 9.60837 2 .4800 .230 .213 9.15155 3 520° .270 .246 8.96181 a. Predictors: (Constant), Schwarz inter-parental conflict scale total b- Predictors: (Constant), Schwarz inter-parental conflict scale total, Mattemal affective quality of relationship C- Predictors: (Constant), Schwarz inter-parental conflict scale total, Mattemal affective quality of relationship, mother as source of support 148 Allowilt Sum of Model Squares df Mean Square F Sig' . 1 Regression 1393.598 1 1393.598 15.095 .000‘ Residual 8401.198 91 92.321 Total 9794.796 92 2 Regression 2257.215 2 1128.607 13.476 .000b Residual 7537.581 90 83.751 Total 9794.796 92 3 Regression 2646.850 3 882.283 10.985 .000° Residual 7147.946 89 80.314 Total 9794.796 92 a. Pmdictors: (Constant), Schwarz inter-parental conflict scale total b- Predictors: (Constant), Schwarz inter-parental conflict scale total, Mattemal affective quality of relationship 0- Predictors: (Constant), Schwarz inter-parental conflict scale total, Mattemal affective quality of relationship, mother as source of support (I. Dependent Variable: tamer as facilitator of independence Coefficient: Unstandardized Standardized Coefficients Coefficients Model B Std. Error Beta t Sig. ‘ 1 (Eonstant) 69.480 1.609 43,173 .000 Schwarz inter-parental conflict scale total -.101 .026 -.377 -3.885 .000 2 (Constant) 52.726 5.438 9,695 .000 Schwarz inter-parental conflict scale total -.099 .025 -.371 -4.007 .000 Mattemal affective ' quality of relationship .168 .052 .297 3.211 .002 3 (Constant) 52.258 5.329 gene .000 Schwarz inter-parental conflict scale total “039 025 --332 -3-596 .001 Mattemal affective quality of relationship .386 .112 .685 3.459 .001 mother as source of support -.386 .175 -.437 -2.203 .030 a. Dependent Variable: father as facilitator of independence 149 Excluded VariablesI Collinearity Partial Statistics ‘ Model Beta In t SIL Correlation Tolerance 1 5,; (i 33?th -.207° -2.174 .032 -223 994 was ”we °f .173“ 1.802 .075 .187 .995 rm'mgip .2978‘ 3.211 .002 .321 1.000 ngicffirm .2538‘ 2.644 .010 .268 .962 2 3'33?“ -.206'D -2.278 .025 -235 .994 mm“ ”um °f -.437b 2203 .030 -227 .208 Sfixéifiigm .099b .795 .429 .084 .557 3 $133:be -.179° -1.981 .051 -207 .969 mmpiizfifm .077c .629 .531 .067 .554 a. Predictors in the Model: (Constant), Schwarz inter-parental conflict scale total b- Predictors in the Model: (Constant), Schwarz inter-parental conflict scale total, Mattemal affecti quality of relationship C- Predictors in the Model: (Constant), Schwarz inter-parental conflict scale total, Mattemal affecti quality of relationship, mother as source of support 0. Dependent Variable: father as facilitator of independence 0.14 Step 4: Father feelings of support was regressed on social desirability, then on maternal feelings of affect, maternal promotion of independence, mother feelings of support, and maternal conflict with father. Model Summary Adjusted Std. Error of Model R R Square R Square the Estimate 1 .2973 .088 .078 14.21765 2 .446” .199 .181 13.39791 3 .516° .266 .241 12.89598 a. Predictors: (Constant), Social Desirability Scale Mean b- Predictors: (Constant), Social Desirability Scale Mean, mother as facilitator of independence 0- Predictors: (Constant), Social Desirability Scale Mean. mother as facilitator of independence, Schwarz inter—parental conflict scale total 150 movnt Sum of Model Squares df Mean Square F Sig. . Wression 1776.303 1 1776.303 8.787 004° Residual 18394.879 91 202.142 Total 20171.183 92 2 Regression 4015.825 2 2007.912 1 1 .186 .000b Residual 16155.358 90 179.504 Total 20171.183 92 3 Regression 5369.915 3 1789.972 10.763 000° Residual 14801268 89 166.306 Total 20171.183 92 a. Predictors: (Constant), Social Desirability Scale Mean b- Predictors: (Constant), Social Desirability Scale Mean, mother as facilitator of independence C- Predictors: (Constant), Social Desirability Scale Mean, mother as facilitator of independence. Schwarz inter—parental conflict scale total d- Dependent Variable: father as source of support Coefficient! Unstandardized Standardized Coefficients Coefficients Model B Std. Error Beta 1 Sig. . 1 (Constant) 54.354 3.139 17.316 .000 Social Desirability Scale Mean -10.114 3.412 -.297 -2.964 .004 2 (Constant) 24.568 8.937 2.749 .007 Social Desirability Scale Mean -10.727 3.220 -.315 -3.332 .001 mother as facilitator of independence .462 .131 .334 3.532 .001 3 (Constant) 34.580 9.290 3.722 .000 Social Desirability Scale Mean -11.305 3.106 -.332 -3.640 .000 mother as facilitator of independence .392 .128 .283 3.055 .003 Schwarz inter-parental conflict scale total -.102 .036 -.265 -2.853 .005 a. Dependent Variable: father as source of support 151 Excluded Variables Collinearity Partial Statistics Model Beta In t Sig. Correlation Tolerance 1 $333338 some °f .166a 1.666 .099 .173 .996 imfiggfi';§’,;j°"°' -.319° -3352 .001 -333 .994 rfigmflraemxs‘fmp .180‘3 1.817 .073 .188 1.000 {fljfpjnfifc‘j'ta‘m °f .334a 3.532 .001 .349 .997 2 $323338 sour” °f -.011° -.102 .919 -011 126 fimflfitggfim -265b 2853 .005 -.290 .958 :‘uifigmg'gmgmp -.058b -.468 .641 -050 .589 3 2132333 source of .0540 .494 .622 .053 .695 Zglifymg'jfigfimp -.013° -.108 .914 -012 .578 3. Predictors in the Model: (Constant), Social Desirability Scale Mean b- Predictors in the Model: (Constant), Social Desirability Scale Mean, mother as facilitator of independence 0 Predictors in the Model: (Constant), Social Desirability Scale Mean, mother as facilitator of independence, Schwarz inter-parental conflict scale total d- Dependent Variable: father as source of support 015 Step 5: Maternal conflict with father was regressed on social desirability, then the three maternal subscales. No significant relations were found. 152 Figure 02 Caucasian Group Normal Probability Plot of the Regression Standardized Residuals of Path Analysis 2 Normal P-P Plot of Regresslon Standardized Residual Dependent Variable: m_tobel 1 .C 0.8- 0.6— 0.4 — ExpectadCumProb 0.2- °-° l l l T 0.0 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1 .0 Observed Cum Prob Figure D3 Caucasian Group Residuals Scatter plot for Path Analysis 2 Scatterplot Dependent Variable: sse_total 2nd 3 o g o o o o o 0 ° 0 14 o t: o o 0 o E d3 0 ° o o 2 0 <5 0 0 ° 2 °‘ ° 5 o 0 9 c at o o o o r: o o s 9 ° 0 K .2— O O O I r I I I '3 -2 -1 o 1 Regression Standardized Predicted Value 153 Table D Caucasian Group Path 2 Decompositions and Calculations of Reproduced Correlations Reproduced r’s Path Decompositions r16 r17 r18 ’19 981 (-.259) (0) 991 + Papas (-.350) + (-.259)(.224) (D) (|) page 1 of 2 r = -.259 = -.332 = -.265 r 27 f 28 P62 (.205) (0) P72 "' r 25 P75 (.685) + (.885)(-.437) ( D) (U) r23 Pas (.640) (.283) (U) P62P96 + r25 P95 (.205)(.224) + (.885)(.377) (I) (U) .181 '37 T23P62 (.640)(.205) (U) rzapm r23P72 (.640)(.685) + (.522)(-.437) (U) (U) Pas (.283) (D) raspes + T32P62P96 (.522)(.377)+ (.640)(.205)(.224) ( U ) (U) 154 = .131 = .211 Ta page 2 of 2 ’46 = P64 = (-.345) = --345 (D) r48 = PM = (-.332) = -.332 (D) r49 = P64P96 = (-.345)(.224) = --077 ( l ) '56 = ''25 P62 = (.885)(.205) = .181 (U) r 57 = P75 + r2:5P72 = (-.437) + (.885)(.685) = .169 (D) (U) r58 : r53p83 = (.522)(.283) = .148 (U) r59 = P95 + r52P62I396 = (.377) + (.885)(.205)+ (.224) = .378 (D) ( U) ’69 = 996 + 961p91+961951 995 = (.224) + (-.259) (-.350) + (-.259) (-.246)(.377) = .339 (D) (S) (S) r 79 = 975995 + P71 P91 + P71P61996 + P72P62P96 + P75r25962996 + P72 r 25995 = (.-.437)(.377) + (-.332)(-.350) + (-.332)(-.259)(.224) *- (.685)(.205)(.224) (S) (S) (S) (S) + (-.437)(.885)(.205)(.224) + (.685)(.885)(.377) 3.230 (3) (S) r 89 = P81P91+ PMPMPss + P81P61P96 + Paaraspes + P83r23P62P96 (-.265)(-.350) + (-.332)(-345)(224) + (-265)(-.259)(.224) + (S) (S) (S (283)(.522)(.377)+ (.283)(.640)(.205)(.224) -- .280 (3) (S) 155 Figure D4 Caucasian Group Path 2 Patemai Promotion of independence M/SUP .. @—fl Well- 5 being 9 Figure D5 Caucasian Group Path 2 Patemai Feelings of Affect M/CON a Weil- bemg WAFF P/AFF 8WD @ Figure DB Caucasian Group Path 2 Paternal Feelings of Support M/ ON 1 bemg Soc/D P/ SUP 4 8 156 APPENDIX E African American and Hispanic Group Path Analysis 2 Figure E1 African American and Hispanic Group Stepwise Multiple Regression for Path Analysis 2 Figure E2 African American and Hispanic group Normal Probability Plot of the Regression Standardized Residuals for Path Analysis 1 Figure E3 African American and Hispanic Residuals Scatter plot for Path Analysis 1 Table E African American and Hispanic Group Path 2 Decompositions and Calculations of Reproduced Correlations Figure E4 African American and Hispanic Group Path 2 Patemai Promotion of Independence Figure E5 African American and Hispanic Group Path 2 Patemai Feelings of Affect Figure E6 African American and Hispanic Group Path 2 Patemai Feelings of Support 157 158 178 179 180 184 184 185 Figure E1 African American and Hispanic Group Stepwise Multiple Regression for Path Analysis 2 E11 Step 1: Well-being was regressed on ethnic identity and social desirability, then on matemai feelings of affect, matemai promotion of independence, mother feelings of support, matemai conflict with father, patemai affective quality of relationship, paternal promotion of independence, and father feelings of support. ' Model Summary Change Statistics Adjusted Std. Error of R Square Model R R Sfluare R Sguare the Estimate Change—I F Change df1 df2 Sig. F Change 1 .543‘ .294 .286 2.26300 .294 35.059 1 84 .000 2 .711” .506 .494 1.90516 .211 35.518 1 83 .000 3 .821c .675 .663 1.55501 .169 42.587 1 82 .000 4 .864“ .746 .733 1 .38264 .071 22.720 1 81 .000 5 .880" .775 .761 1.31009 .029 10.220 1 80 .002 6 .690' .792 .777 1.26596 .018 6.675 1 79 .012 8- Predictors: (Constant). Social Desirability Scale Mean b- Predictors: (Constant), Social Desirability Scale Mean, eth_mean 0- Predictors: (Constant), Social Desirability Scale Mean, eth_mean, Mattemal affective quality of relationship d- Predictors: (Constant), Social Desirability Scale Mean, eth_mean, Mattemal affective quality of relationship, fathe of independence 6- Predictors: (Constant), Social Desirability Scale Mean, eth_mean, Mattemal affective quality of reiationsh'p, fathe of independence, father as source of support f. Predictors: (Constant), Social Desirability Scale Mean, eth_mean, Mattemal affective quality of relationship, father of independence, father as source of support, patemai affective quality of reiationah'o 158 ANOVA‘I Sum of Model Squares df Mean Square F Sig. 1 Regression 179.541 1 179.541 35.059 .000“ Residual 430.178 84 5.121 Total 609.719 85 2 Regression 308.459 2 154.229 42.492 .000b Residual 301 .260 83 3.630 Total 609.719 85 3 Regression 411.438 3 137.146 56.717 .000c Residual 198.281 82 2.418 Total 609.719 85 4 Regression 454.871 4 1 13.718 59.485 .000d Residual 154.848 81 1.912 Total 609.719 85 5 Regression 472.412 5 94.482 55.049 000° Residual 137.307 80 1.716 Total 609.719 85 6 Regression 483.109 6 80.518 50.240 .000f Residual 126.610 79 1.603 Total 609.719 85 a. Predictors: (Constant), Social Desirability Scale Mean b- Predictors: (Constant), Social Desirability Scale Mean, eth_mean G. Predictors: (Constant), Social Desirability Scale Mean, eth_mean, Mattemal affective quality of relationship d- Predictors: (Constant), Social Desirability Scale Mean, eth_mean, Mattemal affective quality of relationship, father as facilitator of independence e. Predictors: (Constant), Social Desirability Scale Mean, eth_mean, Mattemal affective quality of relationship, father as facilitator of independence, father as source of support f- Predictors: (Constant), Social Desirability Scale Mean, eth_mean, Mattemal affective quality of relationship, father as facilitator of independence, father as source of support, patemai affective quality of relationahip 9- Dependent Variable: sse_total 159 Coefficient! Unstandardized Standardized Coefficients Coefficients Model 8 Std. Error Beta t 8L 1 (Constant) 17.124 .724 23.665 .000 gz'gzzamm’ -6.013 1.015 -.543 -5.921 .000 2 (Constant) 8.475 1 .574 5.385 .000 ng'fii‘mwi‘y -5.742 .656 -.518 -6.707 .000 eth_mean 2.586 .434 .460 5.960 .000 3 (Constant) .120 1.814 .066 .947 ggzlgzz'iwrability -3.166 .603 -.286 -3945 .000 eth_mean 2.298 .357 .409 6.438 .000 2'33: m3” .060 .012 .476 6.526 .000 4 (Constant) -.553 1.619 -.341 .734 ggz'figmww -3.005 .714 -.271 4.207 .000 eth_mean 1.952 .326 .348 5.996 .000 :31??er mghip .061 .012 .363 5.256 .000 gaggimm“ °f .057 .012 .303 4.767 .000 5 (Constant) -.659 1.534 -.430 .669 ggzlgzi‘rability -3.242 .661 -.293 -4.761 .000 eth_mean 1.819 .311 .324 5.846 .000 :fift’yms'mgip .061 .01 1 .366 5.569 .000 Eamflmm °f .044 .012 .231 3.604 .001 21%;? ”um °f .036 .011 .167 3.197 .002 6 (Constant) .155 1.516 .102 .919 38:33::be -4.138 .744 -.373 -5.563 .000 eth_mean 2.040 .313 .363 6.525 .000 23;": m§hip .059 .01 1 .354 5.566 .000 mmm" °f .056 .013 .295 4.418 .000 ms ”um °f .093 .024 .481 3.763 .000 fifigagfmgsamp -.046 .016 -.369 -2.584 .012 a. Dependent Variable: sse_total 160 Excluded Variables 9 conflict scale total Model Beta In t Sig_.__ 1 eth_mean .4603 5.960 .000 33:31:: Mimi, .534El 6.051 .000 mmtlm °f .405a 4.650 .000 m; as source of .3103 3.288 .001 fifigflfmfiahm .403‘3 4.813 .000 ngfltammf .5108 6.639 .000 :12; rats source of .3908 4.772 .000 ingfirxama' .1253 1.327 .166 2 rfgisym5'3aefxnp 476° 6.526 .000 {fififlnfiffi'm Of .399b 6.023 .000 31:63:; as source of 355D 4.732 .000 gfltaigaérmzfiamp -279b 3541 .001 $23,197,233?" of .417b 6.060 .000 2163;? source of .304b 4217 .000 afixfxzeml .0460 .566 .558 3 fifig‘pifnzsefi'fim' of 234° 3.099 .003 210$; as source of -.0560 -.453 .651 33273341317334". 226C 3-482 .001 finimanimmf 303° 4.767 .000 films source of .260c 4430 .000 Schwarz inter-parental .040c .604 .547 161 E.1.2 Step 2: Ran same stepwise multiple regression as step 1, excluding mother as source of support, matemai promotion of independence, and matemai conflict with father. R2 and Beta’s of significant variables was unchanged. E.1.3 Step 3: Father feelings of support was regressed on ethnic identity and social desirability, then on matemai feelings of affect, matemai promotion of independence, mother feelings of support, and matemai conflict with father. Model Summary Adjusted Std. Error of Model R R Square R Square the Estimate 1 .2163 .047 .035 13.66470 2 .365b .133 .112 13.11028 3 .437° .191 .162 12.73888 a. Predictors: (Constant), eth_mean b- Predictors: (Constant), eth_mean, mother as facilitator of independence 0- Predictors: (Constant), eth_mean, mother as facilitator of independence, Schwarz inter-parental conflict scale total ANOVA“ Sum of Model Squares df Mean Square F Sig. flagrasion 769.557 1 769.557 4.121 .0463 Residual 1 5684.827 84 186.724 Total 16454384 85 2 Regression 2188.385 2 1094.193 6.366 .003b Residual 14265.999 83 1 71 .880 Total 16454.384 85 3 Regression 3147.506 3 1049.169 6.465 .001° Residual 1 3306.877 82 162.279 Total 16454.384 85 8- Predictors: (Constant), eth_mean b- Predictors: (Constant). eth_mean, mother as facilitator of independence 0- Predictors: (Constant), eth_mean, mother as facilitator of independence, Schwarz inter-parental conflict scale total d- DependentVariable: father as source of support 162 Coefficients" Unstandardized Standardized Coefficients Coefficients Model 8 Std. Error Beta 1 Sig. 1 (Constant) 20.516 10.282 1.995 .049 eth_mean 6.309 3.108 .216 2.030 .046 2 (Constant) -1.052 12.396 -.085 .933 eth_mean 6.109 2.983 .209 2.048 .044 mother as facilitator of independence .351 .122 .294 2.873 .005 3 (Constant) 4.627 12.270 .377 .707 eth_mean 7.205 2.933 .247 2.457 .016 mother as facilitator of i I I noe .299 .121 .250 2.479 .015 Schwarz inter-parental conflict scale total -.098 .040 -.248 -2.431 .017 a. Dependent Variable: father as source of support 163 Excluded Variabled‘ Collinearity Partial Statistics Model Beta In t Sig. Correlation Tolerance 1 332'3§§;“”"‘” .0343 .314 .754 .034 .997 2:333! gmgmp .060“ .745 .459 .031 .932 :mgmgmm °f .2943 2.673 .005 .301 .999 21,13” ”we °f .0453 .418 .677 .046 .997 30m“,;';‘,§';§;'f"‘a' 42938 -2.830 .006 -.297 .976 2 Efiz'figjflab‘m’ .104b .992 .324 .109 .943 glam: gfflsenrp -153" -1.188 .236 -.130 .626 2:11:23: as source of -.0506 -.462 .645 ‘051 .903 ingfir;$?m' -.248b -2.431 .017 -.259 .948 3 gfifi'figzgmbm‘y 166° 1.621 .109 .177 .901 21:1? 3: gimgmp -.154° -1 .234 .221 -.136 .623 $22; as source of 401:: -.948 .346 -.105 .872 8- Predictors in the Model: (Constant), eth_mean b- Predictors in the Model: (Constant), eth_mean, mother as facilitator of independence 0- Predictors in the Model: (Constant), eth_mean, mother as facilitator of independence, Schwarz inter-parental conflict scale total d. DependentVariable: fatheras source ofsupport 164 E.1.4 Step 4: Patemai promotion of independence was regressed on ethnic identity and social desirability, then on matemai feelings of affect, matemai promotion of independence, mother feelings of support, and matemai conflict with father. Model Summary Adjusted Std. Error of Model R R Square R Square the Estimate 1 .2568 .066 .054 13.75966 2 .3448 .116 .097 13.44464 3 .4958 .245 .216 12.51636 3- Predictors: (Constant), eth_mean b- Predictors: (Constant), eth_mean, Social Desirability Scale Mean C- Predictors: (Constant), eth_mean, Social Desirability Scale Mean, mother as facilitator of independence Allovrr‘ Sum of Model Squares df Mean Square F Sig. ‘ 1 Regression 1114.913 1 1114.913 5.889 .0178 Residual 15904.075 84 189.334 Total 17018.988 85 2 Regression 2016.044 2 1006.022 5.577 .0058 Residual 15002.944 83 180.758 Total 17018.988 85 3 Regression 4172.897 3 1390.966 8.879 .0000 Residual 12846.091 82 156.660 Total 17018.988 85 a. Predictors: (Constant). eth_mean '1 Predictors: (Constant), eth_mean, Social Desirability Scale Mean 0 Predictors: (Constant), eth_mean, Social Desirability Sale Mean, mother as facilitator of independence 4 Dependent Variable: father as facilitator of independence 165 Coefficienta‘ Unstandardized Standardized Coefficients Coefficients Model B Std. Error Beta t Sig. 1 (meant) 36.146 10.354 3.491 .001 eth_mean 7.594 3.130 .256 2.427 .017 2 (Constant) 46.382 11.107 4.176 .000 eth_mean 7.232 3.062 .244 2.362 .021 Social Desirability Scale Mean -13.489 6.041 -.230 -2.233 .028 3 (Constant) 15.501 13.273 1.168 .246 eth_mean 7.106 2.851 .239 2.492 .015 Social Desirability Scale Mean -8.735 5.768 -.149 -1.514 .134 mother as facilitator of independence .443 .120 .365 3.710 .000 a. Dependent Variable: father as facilitator of independence 166 Excluded Variables" Collinearity Partial Statistics 1 Model Beta in t Sig._ Correlation Tolerance 1 sz'maxabiw -.230° -2233 .026 -.238 .997 2:66;? gmgmp 396° 4.066 .000 .406 932 mgrg'w“ °f .396. 4.119 .000 .412 .999 $923,” °°°r°° °f .247‘I 2.402 .019 .255 .997 migfimm' -.042° -.396 .693 -.043 .976 2 m“? :vsfixhip 374° 3.312 .001 .343 .745 :L‘geflzsefg'm' °f 365° 3.710 .000 .379 .950 33$,“ °°°°°° °° .164° 1.656 .102 .160 .839 3,122,333?“ 016° .164 .670 .018 .915 3 3:61:96": 323:” 203° 1.499 .136 .164 .495 2:12:12; as source of 095° .881 .381 .097 .789 iffiflfgfl“ 067° .656 .513 .073 .900 a. Predictors in the Model: (Constant), eth_mean b- Predictors in the Model: (Constant), eth_mean, Social Desirability Scale Mean 0 Predictors in the Model: (Constant), eth_mean, Social Desirability Scale Mean, mother as facilitator of independence d- Dependent Variable: father as facilitator of independence E.1.5 Step 5: Repeated step 4 excluding Social desirability. Model Summary Adjusted Std. Error of Model R R Square R Square the Estimate 1 .2568 .066 .054 13.75988 2 .4738 .224 .205 12.61346 3 .511c .262 .235 12.38002 a. Predictors: (Constant), eth_mean b- Predictors: (Constant), eth_mean, mother as facilitator of independence C- Predictors: (Constant), eth_mean, mother as facilitator of independence, Mattemal affective quality of relationship 167 ANOVA‘ Sum of Model Squares df Mean Square F Sig. ‘ 1 Regression 1114.913 1 1114.913 5.889 .0173 Residual 15904075 84 189.334 Total 17018.988 85 2 Regression 3813.692 2 1906.846 11.985 .000b Residual 1 3205.297 83 1 59.1 00 Total 17018.988 85 3 Regression 4451.275 3 1483.758 9.681 .000c Residual 12567.713 82 153.265 Total 17018.988 85 a. Predictors: (Constant), eth_mean b. Predictors: (Constant), eth_mean, mother as facilitator of independence 0- Predictors: (Constant), eth_mean, mother as facilitator of independence, Mattemal affective quality of relationship d- Dependent Variable: tamer as facilitator of independence Coefficient!I Unstandardized Standardized Coefficients Coefficients Model B Std. Error Beta t Sig. 1 (Constant) 36.146 10.354 3.491 .001 eth_mean 7.594 3.130 .256 2.427 .017 2 (Constant) 6.400 1 1 .927 .537 .593 eth_mean 7.318 2.870 .247 2.550 .013 mother as facilitator of independence .484 .117 .398 4.119 .000 3 (Constant) -.081 12.129 -.007 .995 eth_mean 6.450 2.848 .217 2.264 .026 mother as facilitator of independence .307 .144 .253 2.130 .036 Mattemal affective quality of relationship .215 .106 .244 2.040 .045 a. Dependent Variable: father as facilitator of independence 168 Excluded Variables Collinearity Partial Statistics Model M Beta In a t Sig. Correlation Tolerance 1 ffi;’";';,afio;§mp .396 4.066 .000 .406 .982 mgg'mm °f 396° 4.119 .000 .412 .999 52:11:,” °°°°°° °f 247° 2.402 .019 .255 .997 imgiggfi'g‘m' -.042° -.396 .693 -.043 373 2 rufigmslglmfimp 244° 2.040 .045 .220 .628 Sum” °°°°°° °° .138b 1.360 .178 .146 .903 ifflfig'figfi'ggfma' 030° .300 .765 .033 .948 3 $33333 °°m°° °f -.203c -.938 .351 -.104 .192 ifi;‘§§2§§'gfi;§°"‘a' 031° .314 .755 .035 .946 a. Predictors in the Model: (Constant), eth_mean b- Predictors in the Model: (Constant), eth_mean, mother as facilitator of independence C- Predictors in the Model: (Constant), eth_mean, mother as facilitator of independence, Mattemal affective quality of relationship d- Dependent Variable: father as facilitator of independence E.1.6 Step 6: Patemai affective quality of relationship was regressed on ethnic identity and social desirability, then on matemai feelings of affect, matemai promotion of independence, mother feelings of support, and maternal conflict with father. Model Summary Adjusted Std. Error of Model R R Square R Smiare the Estimate 1 .330‘3 .109 .098 19.75561 2 .3948 .156 .135 19.34641 3 .496c .246 .218 18.39827 a. Predictors: (Constant), eth_mean b- Predictors: (Constant), eth_mean, Social Desirability Scale Mean 0- Predictors: (Constant), eth_mean, Social Desirability Scale Mean, Schwarz inter-parental conflict scale total 169 ANOVR’ Sum of Model Squares df Mean Square F Sig. 1 egression 4005.472 1 4005.472 10.263 .0021I Residual 32783.87? 84 390.284 Total 36789.349 85 2 Regression 5723.605 2 2661.903 7.646 .0018 Residual 31 065.544 83 374.284 Total 36789.349 85 3 Regression 9032.663 3 3010.888 8.895 .000c Residual 27756.686 82 338.496 Total 36789.349 85 a. Predictors: (Constant), eth_mean b- Predictors: (Constant), eth_mean, Social Desirability Scale Mean 0- Predictors: (Constant), eth_mean, Social Desirability Scale Mean, Schwarz inter-parental conflict scale total d- Dependent Variable: patemai affective quality of relationahip Coefficient! Unstandardized Standardized Coefficients Coefficients Model B Std. Error Beta 1 Sig._— ‘1'_—_(Constant) 33.102 14.665 2.227 .029 eth_mean 14.394 4.493 .330 3.204 .002 2 (Constant) 47.237 1 5.982 2.956 .004 eth_mean 13.894 4.406 .318 3.153 .002 ggz'gzjgab'my -18.627 6.694 -.216 -2.143 .035 3 (Constant) 46.785 15.200 3.078 .003 eth_mean 16.092 4.249 .369 3.787 .000 gm'gzzgabmw -11.843 6.547 -.138 -1.386 .170 imgigfirgggem' -.185 .059 -.313 -3.127 .002 a. Dependent Variable: patemai affective quality of relationahip 170 Excluded Variables Collinearity Partial Statistics Model Beta In t Sig. Correlation Tolerance 1 $03,333:?“ -.216° -2.143 .035 -.229 .997 rfim'mEhip 209° 2.051 .043 .220 .982 fimgfijmm °° 263° 2.646 .010 .279 .999 m” °°°°°° °° .161° 1.579 .116 .171 .997 33:22:35?“ -.349° -3.576 .001 -655 .973 2 f;?§;"§';f;f§;§mp .135° 1.153 .252 .126 .745 {flgfp‘fnzifi'm' °° 226° 2.241 .026 .240 .950 31%;” °°°°°° °° 069° .611 .420 .069 .639 mmzrgfifm' -.313° -3.127 .002 -.326 .915 3 ruztliiymtfrlrflfaiichsenip “1° 1-271 207 .140 .744 $$§22£fl°°°°° °° .190° 1.949 .055 .212 .935 $3,” °°°°°° °° 043° .405 .666 .045 .621 a. Predictors in the Model: (Constant), eth_mean b- Predictors in the Model: (Constant), eth_mean, Social Desirability Scale Mean 0- Predictors in the Model: (Constant), eth_mean, Social Desirability Scale Mean, Schwarz inter-parental conflict scale total ‘1 Dependent Variable: paternal affective quality of relationahip 171 E.1.7 Step 7: Ran step 6 excluding social desirability. Model Summary Adjusted Std. Error of Model R R Square R Sgare the Estimate 1 .330‘3 .109 .098 19.75561 2 .477b .228 .209 18.49994 3 520° .270 .243 18.09697 a. Predictors: (Constant), eth_mean b- Predictors: (Constant), eth_mean, Schwarz inter-parental conflict scale total c. Predictors: (Constant), eth_mean, Schwarz inter-parental conflict scale total, mother as facilitator of independence ANOVA“ Sum of Model Squares df Mean Square F Sig. ‘ 1 Regression 4005.472 1 4005.472 10.263 .002a Residual 32783.877 84 390.284 Total 36789.349 85 2 Regression 8382.797 2 4191 .398 12.247 .000b Residual 28406.552 83 342.248 Total 36789.349 85 3 Regression 9934.309 3 3311.436 10.111 000’ Residual 26855.039 82 327.500 Total 36789.349 85 a. Predictors: (Constant), eth_mean b- Predictors: (Constant), eth_mean, Schwarz inter-parental conflict scale total C- Predictors: (Constant), eth_mean, Schwarz inter-parental conflict scale total, mother as facilitator of independence (1 Dependent Variable: patemai affective quality of relationahip 172 Coefficbnta‘ Unstandardized Standardized Coefficients Coefficients Model B Std. Error Beta t Si . 1 (Constant) 33.102 14.865 2.227 .029 eth_mean 14.394 4.493 .330 3.204 .002 2 (Constant) 38.327 13.997 2.738 .008 eth_mean 16.637 4.254 .381 3.911 .000 Schwarz inter-parental flict scale total -.205 .057 -.349 -3.576 .001 3 (Constant) 14.849 17.430 .852 .397 eth_mean 16.184 4.167 .371 3.884 .000 Schwarz inter-parental conflict scale total -.183 .057 -.311 -3.212 .002 mother as facilitator of independence .373 .171 .209 2.177 .032 3- Dependent Variable: patemai affective quality of relationahip Excluded Variabied‘ Collinearity Model r Beta in t Sig. Correlation Tolerance 1 Mattemal affective a quality of relationship .209 2.051 .043 .220 .982 mother as facilitator of a independence .263 2.646 .010 .279 .999 mother as source of a support .161 1.579 .118 .171 .997 Schwarz inter-parental a conflict scale total -.349 -3.576 .001 -.365 .978 2 Mattemal affective b quality of relation ship .173 1.792 .077 .194 .970 mother as facilitator of b independence .209 2.177 .032 .234 .968 mother as source of b support .087 .874 .385 .096 .944 3 Mattemal affective c quality of relationship .076 .637 .526 .071 .628 mother as source of c support .031 .301 .764 .033 .872 a. Predictors in the Model: (Constant), eth_mean b- Predictors in the Model: (Constant), eth_mean, Schwarz inter-parental conflict sale total C- Predictors in the Model: (Constant), eth_mean, Schwarz inter-parental conflict scale total, mother as facilitator of independence ‘1 Dependent Variable: paternal affective quality of relationahip 173 E.1.8 Step 8: Ethnic identity was regressed on social desirability, then on matemai affective quality of relationship, maternal promotion of independence, mother as source of support, maternal conflict with father, patemai feelings of affect, father promotion of independence, and father feelings of support. Model Summary Adjusted Std. Error of Model R R Square R Square the Estimate 1 .3303 .109 .098 .45286 2 .417b .174 .154 .43663 a. Predictors: (Constant), patemai affective quality of relationahip b- Predictors: (Constant), patemai affective quality of relationahip, Schwarz inter-parental conflict scale total ANOVR= Sum of Model Stuares df Mean Square F Si . 1 Regression 2.105 1 2.105 10.263 .002a Residual 17.227 84 .205 Total 19.331 85 2 Regression 3.363 2 1.661 6.739 .0008 Residual 15.969 83 .192 Total 19.331 85 a. Predictors: (Constant), patemai affective quality of relationahip b. Predictors: (Constant), patemai affective quality of relationahip, Schwarz inter-parental conflict scale total C- Dependent Variable: eth_mean Coefficient! Unstandardized Standardized Coefficients Coefficients Model B Std. Error Beta t Sig. ‘ 1 (Constant) 2.667 .196 13.635 .000 patemai affective quality of rel ati on ahip .008 .002 .330 3.204 .002 2 (Constant) 2.303 .237 9.721 .000 patemai affective quality of relati on ahip .009 .002 .408 3.911 .000 Schwarz inter-parental co nfli ct scale total .004 .001 .267 2.557 .012 3- Dependent Variable: eth_mean 174 Excluded Vel'leblei Collinearity pamaj Statistics Beta In t Sig. Correlation Tolerance Scaz'MeaL'a my 025° .236 .612 .026 .946 fifigms'rmxgp 055° .512 .610 .056 .936 fixefenfacim" °f -071° -.663 .509 -073 .927 3:32” ”me °° -.106° -1023 .309 -.112 .960 :megzcigfggfe'fma' 267° 2.557 .012 .270 .914 mic???“ °° .109° .690 .376 .097 .706 $2322” ”um °° -.319° -1.495 .139 -.162 .230 332:: 3221301013) -025° -.236 .814 -.026 .912 mfig'm3m 059° .569 .571 .063 .937 mnzg'w" °° -045° -.430 .668 -047 .917 mg” ”me °f -056° -.543 .588 -.060 .939 :g‘eifiginam °f 053° .433 .666 .046 .660 $23228 °°m°° °° -.321° -1557 .123 -.170 .230 a. Predictors in the Model: (Constant), patemai affective quality of relationahip b. Predictors in the Model: (Constant), patemai affective quality of relationahip, Schwarz inter-parental conflict scale total 0- Dependent Variable: eth_mean E.1.9 Step 9: Social desirability was regressed on ethnic identity, then on matemai affective quality of relationship, matemai promotion of independence, mother as source of support, matemai conflict with father, patemai feelings of affect, father promotion of independence, and father feelings of support. 175 Model Summary Adjusted Std. Error of Model R R Square R Square the Estimate 1 .494£‘ .244 .235 .21 143 2 .533” .284 .267 .20698 a. Predictors: (Constant), Mattemal affective quality of relationship *1 Predictors: (Constant), Mattemal affective quality of relationship, Schwarz inter-parental conflict scale total ANOVAc Sum of Model Squares df Mean Square F Sig. 1 Regression 1.211 1 1.211 27.093 .0003 Residual 3.755 84 .045 Total 4.966 85 2 Regression 1.410 2 .705 16.460 .0008 Residual 3.556 83 .043 Total 4.966 85 a. Predictors: (Constant), Mattemal affective quality of relationship b- Predictors: (Constant), Mattemal affective quality of relationship, Schwarz inter-parental conflict scale total 0- Dependent Variable: Social Desirability Scale Mean Coefficients| Unstandardized Standardized Coefficients Coefficients Model B Std. Error Beta t S' . 1 (Constant) 1 .379 .1 38 9.995 .000 Mattemal affective quality of relationship -.007 .001 -.494 -5.205 .000 2 (Constant) 1 .270 . 144 8.797 .000 Mattemal affective quality of relationship -.007 .001 -.476 -5.107 .000 Schwarz inter-parental conflict scale total .001 .001 .201 2.156 .034 a. Dependent Variable: Social Desirability Scale Mean 176 Excluded VariabieG support Collinearity Partial Statistics Model Beta In t Sig. Correlation Tolerance 1 eth_mean .0133 .137 .891 .015 .982 mother as facilitator of a inl l .113 .953 .344 .104 .642 mother as source of a support .077 .437 .663 .048 .290 Schwarz inter-parental a conflict scale total .201 2.156 .034 .230 .992 patemai affective a quality of rel ati on ahip -.117 -1.201 .233 -.131 .938 father as facilitator of a independence -.041 -.390 .698 -.O43 .820 father as source of a support .076 .797 .428 .087 .988 2 eth_mean -.020b -.208 .836 -.O23 .957 mother as facilitator of b independence .154 1.316 .192 .144 .628 mother as source of support .207 1.149 .254 .126 .264 patemai affective 9 quality of rel ati on ahip -.064 -.642 .523 -.O71 .864 father as facilitator of b independence -.049 -.479 .634 -.053 .819 a father as ”m” °f .134 1.401 .165 .153 .926 a. Predictors in the Model: (Constant), Mattemal affective quality of relationship b- Predictors in the Model: (Constant), Mattemal affective quality of relationship, Schwarz inter-parental conflict scale total C- Dependent Variable: Social Desirability Scale Mean 177 Figure E2 African American and Hispanic group Normal Probability Plot of the Regression Standardized Residuals for Path Analysis 1 Normal P-P Plot of Regression Standardized Residual Dependent Variable: sse_total Expected Cum Prob 0-07 i i l r 0.0 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 Observed Cum Prob 178 Figure E3 African American and Hispanic Residuals Scatter plot for Path Analysis 1 Scatterplot Dependent Variable: sse_total 3— To 3 0 3 o 3 24 o o l: o o 3 g 1" oo o o a Q: ° 0 1a O E O 0 ° 00 O 0- O O O 0:) o o o 8 .2 o 3 _1_ 0 ° o g o o o o O n: o 2_ o l I i i I -3 -2 -1 0 1 Regression Standardized Predicted Value 179 Table E African American and Hispanic Group Path 2 Decompositions and Reproduced r’s r14 r15 r16 r17 r18 r19 Calculations of Reproduced Coglations page 1 of 4 Path Decompositions r P41 (--476) = -.476 (D) r12P62P65 (.598) (.29) (.408) = .051 (U) P61 + r12P62 (.244) + (.598) (.253) = .395 (D) (U) T12P12 (.598) (.250) = .150 (U) T12P82 (.598) (.209) = .124 (U) P91 "' P61P96 + P41P94 + r12P62P96 + r12P62P65P95 + r12P62P65P75P67 1’ T12P62P65P65P96 "’ r12P62P96 "' r12P72P97 683 (.354) + (.244)(.295 ) + (-.476)(-.373) + (.596)(.209)(-.369) (D) ( l ) ( l ) (U) + (.598)(.209)(.408)(.363)+ (.598)(.209)(.408)(.247)(.481) (U) (U) + (.598)(.209)(.408)(.217)(.295) + (.596)(.253)(.295) U (U) ( ) + (.598)(.250)(.480) (U) r 24 r 25 r 26 r27 r28 r12P42 (.598)(-.476) = -.285 (U) P62P65 (.209)(.408) = .065 (|) P62 + P62P65P65 + r12P62 (.253) + (.209)(.408)(.217) + (.598)(.244) = .417 (D) (l ) (U) P72 ‘* P62P65P75 (.250) + (.209)(.408)(.247) = .271 (D) (|) P62 (209) = .209 (D) 180 DQQEZOf4 r29 = P62P96+P62P65P95+P62P65P75P97+P62P65P65P96+P62P96+P72P97 + r12P91 'l' r12P61P96 T r12P41P94 = (.209)(-.369) + (.209)(.408)(.363) + (.209)(.408)(.247)(.481) ( I ) ( l ) ( | ) + (.209)(.408)(.217)(.295)+ (.253)(.295) + (.250)(.481) ( l ) ( l ) ( l ) + (.598)(.354) + (.598)(.244)(.295) + (.598)(-.476)(-.373) = .525 (U) (U) U) ’34 = P43 = (.201) = .201 (D) r35 = P53 "’ P63P65 = (.267) + (-.31)(.408) = .140 (D) ( l ) r35 = P53P65 + P63P65P65 = (.267)(.217) + (-.311)(.408)(.217) =.030 ( l ) ( l ) f 37 = P73 + P53P75 4’ P63P65P75 = (.248) + (.267)(.247) + (-.311)(.408)(.247) = -.2‘l3 (D) ( l ) ( l ) r38 = P63 = (-.311) = -.311 (D) r39 = P43P94 T P73Ps7 + P53P95 "' P53P75P97 "’ P35P56P96 T P63P96 "' P63P65P95 + P63P65P75P97 + P63P65P65P96 = (.201)(-.373) + (-.248)(.481)(.267)(.363) + (.267)(.247)(.481) ( l ) ( l ) l + (.267)(.217)(.295) + (-.311)(-.369) + (-.311)(.408)(.363) ( | ) ( | ) ( l ) + (-.311)(.408)(.247)(.481) + (-.311)(.408)(.217)(.295) = -.003 ( r ) ( I ) '45 = P43P54 + P43P63P65 + P41 (12 P62P65 = (.201)(.267) + (.201)(-.311)(.408) + (-.476)(.598)(.209)(.408) = .004 (S) (S) (S) ’46 = P43P53P65 + P41P61 "’ P43P63P65P65 1’ P41r12P62 + P41T21P62P65P65 = (.201)(.267)(.217) + (-.476)(.244) + (.201)(-.311)(.408)(.217) (S) (S) (S) + (-.476)(.598)(.253)+ (-.476)(.598)(.209) = -.187 (S) (S) ’47 P43P73 7’ P43P53P75 1' P43P63P65P75 + P41f12P72 + P41712P62P65P75 (.201)(—.248) + (.201)(.267)(.247) + (.201)(-.311)(.408)(.247) s (S) (S) ( ) + (-.476)(.598)(.252) + (-.476)(.596)(.209)(.406)(.247) = -.120 (S) (S) 181 IL! E page 3 of 4 r43 = P43P63 T P41|'12P62 = (.201)(-.311) + (-.476)(.598)(.209) = -.122 (S) (S) ’49 = P94 T P43P73P97 T P43P53P95 T P43P63P96T P43P63P65P95 T P43P63P65P75P97 T P43P53P75P97 T P41P91 TP41P61P96 T P41712P72P97 TP41T12P62P96 T P41712P62P96 T P41r12P62P65P95 T P41712P62P65P57P97 604 = (-.373)+ (.201)(-.248)(.481) + (.201)(.267)(.363) + (.201)(-.311)(-.369) (D) (S) (S) (S) + (.201)(-.311)(.408)(.363) + (.201)(-.311)(.408)(.247)(.481) (S) (S) + (.201)(.267)(.247)(.481) + (-.476)(.354) + (-.476)(.244)(.295) (S) (S) (S) + (-.476)(.598)(.250)(.481) + (-476)(.598)(.253)(.295) (S) (S + (-.476)(.598)(.209)(-.369) + ( -.476)(.598)(.209)(.408)(.363) (S) (S) +(-.476)(.598)(.209)(.408)(247)(.481)+(-.476)(.598)(.209)(.408)(.217)(.295) (S) (S) ’56 = P65 = (.217) = .217 (D) r57 = P75 = (.247) = .247 r58 = P85 T P53P63 = (.408) + (.267)(-.311) = -.325 (D) (S) r59 = P95 T P75P97 T 965me Despite T P53P43P94 T P53P63P96 = (.363) +(.247)(.481) + (.408)(-.369) +(.217)(.295) +(.267)(-.248)(.481) (D) ( | ) (S) ( I ) (S) + (2.67)(.201)(-.373) + (.267)(-.311)(-.369) . .374 (S) (S) ’69 = P96 T P62P26P65P95 T P62P62P65P75P97 T P62P72P97 T P62P62P96 T P62r12P91 T P62712P41P97 T P61P91 T P61P41P94 T P61r12P72P97 T P61r12P62P96 T P61712P62P65P95 T P61r12P62P65P75P97 ' 539 (.295) + (.253)(.209)(.406)(.363) + (.253)(.209)(.408)(.247)(.481) (D) (S) (S) + (.253)(.250)(.481) + (.253)(.209)(-.369) + (.253)(.596)(.354) s s s ( ) ( ) ( ) + (.253)(.598)(-.476)(-.373) + (.244)(.354) + (2.44)(-.476)(-.373) (S) (S) (S) + (.244)(.598)(.250)(.481) + (.244)()(.596)(.209)(-.369) s s ( ) ( ) +(.244)(.596)(.209)(.406)(.363)+ (.244)(.596)(.209)(.406)(.247)(.461) (S) (S) 182 Table E page 4 of 4 ’79 = P97 T P75P95 T P75P65P96T PnP53P95T P73P53P65P96T P73P63P65P95 T P73P63P96 T P72P62P96 TP73P63P65P65P96 T P72P62P65P95 T P72P62P65P65P95 T P72I12P61P96+ P72F12P91 T P72I12P41P94 = (.481)+(.247)(.363)+(247)(.217)(.295)+(-.248).267)(.363) +(-.248)(-.31 1)(-.369) (D) (S) (S) (S) (S) + (-.248)(.267)(.217)(.295) + (-.248)(-.311)(.408)(.363) + (.250)(.209)(-.369) S S (S) ( ) ( ) + (-.248)(-.311)(.408)(.217)(.295)+ (.250)(.209)(.408)(.363) (S) S + (.250)(.209)(.408)(.217)(.265) + (.250)(.598)(.244)(.295) S (S) + (.250)(.596)(.354) + (.250)(.598)(-.476)(-.373) = .457 (S) (S) P96 T P65P95 T P65P75P97 T P62P62P96 T P62P72P97 T P62I12P91 T P62T12P61P96 T P62r12P41P94 T P60P53P95 T P63P53P75P97 T P63P73P97 T P63P53P65P96 (-.369) + (.406)(.363) + (.408)(.249)(.481) + (.408)(.217)(.295) (D) (I ) ( I ) (I ) + (.209)(.253)(.295) +(.209)(.250)(.481) + (.209)(.596)(.354) (S) (S) S) + (.209)(.598)(.244)(.295) + (.209)(.598)(-.476)(-373) (S) (S) r 69 + (-.311)(.201)(-.373) + (-.311)(.267)(.363) + (- .311)(.267)(.247)(.481) (S) (S) (S) + (-3.11)(.267)(.217)(.295) + (-.311)(-.248)(.481) -- .117 (S) (S) 183 M/AFF I i I -.476** SocD 4 .244‘ WELL- BEING M/IND 201 t l 9 ' m8“) . 95" 2 .2538 M/c-QN ETH 1 + Figure E4 African American and Hispanic Group Path 2 Patemai Promotion of independence .0 , l 4: all WELL- -.476 BEING M/iND 2 MICON 3 Figure E5 African American and Hispanic Group Path 2 Patemai Feelings of Affect 184 Soc D a WELL- BEING : .48 I " M/iND 25° P/SUP @ .0» 7 ETH 1 WC ON > 5 t 3 @ Figure E6 African American and Hispanic Group Path 2 Father Feelings of Suppon 185 References Adams, T. A., Bezner, J. B., Drabbs, M. E. (2000). Conceptualization and measurement of the spiritual and psychological dimensions of wellness in a college population. Journal of American College Health. 4c8 (4). 165-173. Adams, T.A., Bezner J. B., Steinhardt M. A. (1997). The conceptualization and measurement of wellness: Integrating balance across and within dimensions. American Joml of Health Promotion;11(3). 208-218. Ahrons, C. (1994). The Good Divorce. New York, New York: Harper Collins Publisher. Ainsworth, M. D. S. (1989). Attachments beyond infancy. American Psycholpgist, 44, 709-716. Ainsworth, M. D. S. & Bowlby, J. (1991). An ethological approach to personality development. American Psychologist. 46, 333-341. Amato, P.R. & Gilbreth, JG. (1999). Nonresident fathers and Children's well-being: a meta analysis. Journal of Marriage & FalImilv. 61, 557-573. Amato PR, Booth A (1996), A prospective study of divorce and parent- child relationships. Journal of Marriage and Famiu, 58, 356-365. Amato, P. R. (1993). Children's adjustment to divorce: Theories, hypotheses, and empirical support. Journal of Marriage and the Family 55, 23- 38. Amato, P. R., & Keith, B. (1991). Parental Divorce and the Well-being of Children: A Meta-analysis. Psychoipgical Bulletin, 110, 26-46. Ardell D. (1986) High Level Wellness_. Ten Speed Press; Berkeley, CA. Arditti, J. A. & Prouty, A. M. ( 1999). Change, disengagement, and renewal: relationship dynamics between young adults and their fathers after divorce. Journal of Marital and Family Therapy, 25, 61-81. Arnstein, R. (1980). The student, the family, the university, and the transition to adulthood. Adolescent Psychiatg, 8, 160-172. Arnsden, G. C., & Greenberg, M. T. (1987). The inventory of parent and peer attachment: Individual differences and their relationship to psychological well-being in adolescence. Journal of Youth and Adolescence 16 427-454. 186 Asendorpf, J. B., 81 Wilpers, S. (2000). Attachment security and available support: Closely linked relationship qualities. Jaamal of Social and Personal Relationships. 17. 1 15-138. Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., van lJendoom, M. H., & Kroonenberg, P. M. (2004). Differences in attachment security between African American and white children: Ethnicity or socio-economic status? Infant Behavior and Development, 27, 41 7-433. Baldwin, M. W., & Fehr, B. (1995). On the instability of attachment style ratings. Personal Relationships, 2, 247-261. Barnes, G. G., (1999). Divorce transitions: Identifying risk and promoting resilience for children and their parental relationships. flrnal of Marital anq Family Therapy, 25 (4), 425-441. Bartholomew, K., & Horowitz, L. M. (1991). Attachment style among young adults: A test of a four-category model. flimal of PersonalihLand Social Psychology. 61. 226-244. Belsky, J., Campbell, S. B., Cohn, J. F., & Moore, G. (1996). Instability of infant-parent attachment security. Developmental Psvchoquy, 32, 921-924. Billingham, R. E. & Abrahams, T. (1998). Parental divorce, body dissatisfaction and physical attractiveness ratings of self and others among college women. Collage Student Journal, 32, 148-152. Billingsley, A. (1990). Understanding African American family diversity. In J. Dewart (Ed.), The State of Black America 1990, New York: National Urban League. Brack, G, Gay, M. F ., Matheny, K. B. (1993). Relationships between attachment and coping resources among late adolescents. Journal of Coll_e_ge Stuciant development. May. 34 (3). 212-215. Blain, M., Thompson, J., & Whiffen, V. (1993). Attachment and perceived social support in late adolescence: The interaction between working models of self and others. Jol_lrnal of flolescent Research. 8. 226-241. Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss: Vol. 1. Attachment. London: Hogarth. Bradford, E., Lyddon, W. J. (1993). Current parental attachment: Its relation to perceived psychological distress and relationship satisfaction in college students. Journal of College Stadent Qavelopment. 34 (3). 256-260. 187 Braver, S., Wolchik, S., Sandler, l., Fogas, B., & Svetina, D. (1991). Frequency of visitation by divorced fathers: Differences in reports by fathers and mothers. American Journal of Qrthopgivchiatrvfit 448-454. Carranza, L. V. & Kilmann, P. R. (2000). Links between perceived parent characteristics and attachment variables for young women from intact families. Aaolescence. 35 (138) 295-312. Cavell, T., Jones, D., Runyan, R. D., Constantin-Page, L., 8. Velasquez, J. (1993). Perceptions of attachment and adjustment of adolescents with alcoholic fathers. Jaumal of Family Psvcholqgv. 7, 204-212. Cemkovich, S. A., & Giordano, P. C. (1987). Family relationships and delinquency. Criminolagy, 25, 295-321. Chapman, L. S. (1987). Developing a useful perspective on spiritual health: Well-being, spiritual potential and the search for meaning. American Journal of Health Pronflon, 1(3), 31-39. Chickering, A. W. (1969). Education and identfiy‘ . San Francisco: Jossey- Bass. Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psycholpgical Bulletin, 112, 155-159. Cook, W. L. (1994). A structural equation model of dyadic relationships with the family system. flirnal of Consglting and_Clinical Psychology. 6_2_, 500- 509. Crawley, R. (1988). Black families in a neo-conservative era. Famiy Relations. 37. 415-419. Crowne, D. P., & Marlow, D. (1960). A new scale of social desirability independent of psychopathology. Journal of Consult Psvcholpgy, 24, 349-335. Cummings, E. M., & Watson-O’Reilly, A. (1997). Fathers in family context: Effects of marital quality on child adjustment. In M. E. Lamb (Ed.), The role of t_he fa_ther in child development (3rd ed). New York: Wiley. D’Angelli, A. R. & Hershberger, S. L. (1993). African American undergraduates on a predominantly White campus: Academic factors, social networks, and campus climate. Journal of Negro Education, 62, 67-81. Das Eiden, R., Teti, D. M., 8 Corns, K. M. (1995). Maternal working models of attachment, marital adjustment, and the parent-child relationship. Child Developmem, 66, 1504-1518. 188 Davies, P. T., & Cummings, E. M., (1994). Marital conflict and child adjustment: An emotional security hypothesis. Psychological Bulletin, 116, 387- 41 1. DeCindio, L. A., Floyd, H. H., Wilcox, J. A., McSevency, (1983). Race effects in a model of parent-peer orientation. lI_tr_Ioles_c_ence, 18, 369-379. Depken D. (1994) Wellness through the lens of gender: A paradigm shift. Wellness Perspectives, 10, 54-69. Dewitt-Parker, J. (2000). Predicting the successful adjustment of black freshman to a predominantly white university: The contribution of parental attachment and cultural traditionality. Dissertation-Abstracts International Section A: Humanities and Social Sciences, 60 (8-A), 2812. Donley, M. G. (1993). Attachment and the emotional unit. Family Process, Q, 3-20. Dombusch, S., Carlsmith, J., Bushwall, S., Ritter, P., Leiderman, H., Hastorf, A., & Gross, R. (1985). Single parents, extended households, and the control of adolescents, Child Development, 56, 326-341. Doyle, A. B., Markiewicz, M. B., Lieberman, M., & Voss, K. (2000). Child attachment security and self-concept: Associations with mother and father attachment style and marital quality. Merrill-Palmer Quartem, 46 (3), 514-539. Dudley, J. R. (1991). The consequences of divorce proceedings for divorced fathers. Journal of Divorce and Remarriage, 16, 171-193. Duesterhaus, M. E. B. (2004). The predictive value of college students’ attachment styles on the development of intrinsically or extrinsically motivated life goal. Dissertation Abstract International Section-A: Humanities and Social Sciences, 65 (5-A), 1688. Dunn, H.L. (1966) High-level wellness for man and society. In: FoItaJR, Deck ES, eds. A sociolpgical framework for patient care. New York: John Wiley & Sons, 213-219. Egeland, B. & Farber, E. a. (1984). Infant-mother attachment: Factors related to its development and changes over time. Child Development, 55, 753- 771. Elicker, J., Englund, M., & Stroufe, L. A. (1992). Predicting peer competence and peer relationships in childhood from early parent-child relationships. In R. Parke & G. Ladd (Eds), Famiu-gr relations: Modes of linkage (77-106). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. 189 Emery, R. E., & Forehand, R. (1994). Parental divorce and children’s well-being: A focus on resilience. In R. J. Haggerty, L. R. Sherrod, N. Garmezy & M. Rutter (Eds), imss. risk. and_rasilience in chflren and adolescents: Processes. mechanisms and interventions (pp. 64-99). New York: Cambridge University Press. Erel, 0., 8. Burman, B. (1995). lnterrelatedness of marital relations and parent-child relations: A meta-analytic review. Psycholpgical Bulletin, 118, 108- 1 32. Famworth, M. (1984). Family structure, family attributes, and delinquency in a sample 0 flow-income, minority males and females. Journal of Youth and A_dolescence. 13, 349-364. Feldman, S. S., & Eliott, G. R. (1990). At the threshold: The developipg adolescent. Cambridge, MA. Harvard University Press. Florian, V., Mikulincer, M., 8 Bucholtz, I. (1995). Effects of adult attachment style on the perception and search for social support. Journal of Psvchologv. 12a, 665-675. Fox, N. A., Kimmerly, N. L., & Schafer, W. D. (1991). Attachment to mother/ attachment to father: A meta-analysis. Child Development, 62, 210-225. Furstenberg, F. F., Morgan, S. P., & Allison, P. D. (1987). Parental participation and children's well-being after marital dissolution. American Sociolpgical Review, 52, 695-701. Gallagher, M. (1996). The Abolition of Marriage. Regnery Publisher. Giordano, P. C., Cemkovich, S. A., & DeMaris, A. (1993). Family and peer relations of Black adolescents. Journal of Marriage and the Famiy, 55, 277-287. Goldberg, W. A. (1990). Marital quality, parental personality, and spousal agreement about perceptions and expectations of children. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 36, 531-556. Gove, W. 8 Shin, H. (1989). The psychological well-being of divorced and widowed men and women: An empirical analysis. Jofiumal of Family Issqes. 10. 122-144. Grayson, P. (1998). A fast and bumpy ride: Students and therapy in an ever changing environment. Journal of College Student anchotherapy, 13, 3-13. 190 Greenberg J. S. (1985) Health and wellness: A conceptual differentiation. Journal of School Health 55 403-406. Griffin, D., & Bartholomew, K. (1994). Models of the self and other: Fundamental dimensions underlying measures of adult attachment. Journal of Personality & Social Psvchologv. 67. 430-445. Grossman, K. E., Grossman, K., Huber, F., & Warner, U. (1981) German children’s behavior towards their mothers at 12 months and their fathers at 18 months in Ainsworth’s strange situation. International Jogrnal of Behavioral Development, 4, 157-181 . Grotevant, H. D. (1989). Child development within the family context. In W. Damon (Ed), Child development today and tomorrow (34-51). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Grotevant, H. D., & Cooper, R. (1986). Individuation in family relationships. Human Development, 29, 82-100. Hannum, J. W. & Dvorak, D. M. (2004). Effects of family conflict, divorce, and attachment patterns on the psychological distress and social adjustment of college freshmen, Journal of Collage Student Development, 45(1), 27-42. Hanson, T. L. ( 1999). Does parental conflict explain why divorce is negatively associated with child welfare? Social Forces, 77 (4), 1283-316. Harrison, A. 0., Wilson, M. N., Pine, C. J., Chan, S. Q., & Buriel, R. (1990). Family ecologies of ethnic minority children. Child Development, 61, 347- 362. Hayashi, G. M & Strickland, B. R. (1998). Long-terrn effects of parental divorce on love relationships: Divorce as attachment disruption. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 15 (1) 23-38. Harris, I. & Salt, R.(1999) Patterns of paternity. Journal of Men’s Studies 7 (2), 245-264. Hazan, C., 8. Shaver, P. R. (1994). Attachment as an organizational framework for research on close relationships. Psycholagical Ingt_lirv. 5 (1). 1-22. Hazan, C., & Shaver, P. R. (1987). Romantic love conceptualized as an attachment process. Journal of Personalfly’ ad Social Psychology. 59, 270-280. Heatherton, T.F. & Polivy, J. (1991). Development and validation of a scale for measuring state self-esteem. Journal of Parsonalitv and Social Pa ycholggy, 60, 895-910. 191 Hetherington, E. M. (1992). Coping with marital transitions: A family systems perspective. In E. M. Hetherington & W. G. Clingempeel (Eds) Coping with marital transitions (1-14). Hetherington, E.M., Cox, M. & Cox, R. (1982) Effects of divorce on parents and children. In: M. Lamb (Ed.), Nontracflonal families: Parenting ang child development. Hilldale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Hettler, W. (1984) Wellness: Encouraging a lifetime pursuit of excellence. Health Values. 8 (4), 13-17. Hinderlie, H. H., Kenny, M. (2002). Attachment, social support, and college adjustment among black students at predominantly white universities. Journal of Collage Student Development, 43 (3), 327-340. Hines, A. (1997). Divorce-related transitions adolescent development, and the role of the parent-child relationship: A review of the literature. Journal of Marriage and the Family. 59, 375-388. Hojat, M. (1998). Satisfaction with early relationships with parents and psychosocial attributes in adulthood: Which parent contributes more?, Journal (if—Genetic Psychology, 159 (2), 203-220. Holmbreck, G. N., & Wandrei, M. (1993). Individual and relational predictors of adjustment in first-year college students. Journal of @nseling PsmhologyAO, 73—78. Hughes, M., & Demo, D. H. (1989). Self-perceptions of Black Americans: Self-esteem and personal efficacy. American Journal of Sociology, 95, 132-159. Ihinger-Tallman, M., Pasley, K., & Buehler, C. (1993). Developing a middle-range theory of father involvement postdivorce. Journal of Family Issues, 141, 550-571. Jekielek, S. M. ( 1998). Parental conflict, marital disruption and children's emotional well-being. SLocfil Force_§. 76. (3), 905-36. Kelly, J. B. (2000). Children's adjustment in conflicted marriage and divorce: A decade review of research. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psvchiatm, 39 (8), p. 963-73. Kelly, J. B. (1988). Longer-term adjustment in children of divorce: Converging finding and implications for practice. Journal of Family Psychology, _2_, 1 19-140. 192 Kenny, M. E. (1990). College seniors' perceptions of parental attachments: The value and stability of family ties. Journal of Collage Studenthvelopment. 3_1, 3946. Kenny, M. E. (1987). The extent and function of parental attachment among first-year college students. Journal of Youth and Adolesgnce. 16. 17-29. Kenny, M. E. (1987). The parental attachment questionnaire. (Available from M. Kenny, Boston College.) Kenny, M. E., & Donaldson, G. A. (1991). Contributions of parental attachment and family structure to the social and psychological functioning of first-year college students. Journal of Counseling Psycholpgy, 38, 479-486. Kenny, M. E, Gallagher, L. A., Alvarez, S. R., & Silsby, J. (2002). Sources of support and psychological distress among academically successful inner-city youth. Adfolescence. 37 (145). 161-182. Kenny, M. E., Lomax, R., Brabeck, M., & Fife, J. (1998). Longitudinal pathways linking adolescent reports of maternal and paternal attachment to psychological well-being. Journal of Early Adolescence, 18(3), 221-243. Kenny, M. E. & Perez, V. (1996) Attachment and psychological well-being among racially and ethnically diverse first-year college students. Journal of CollegStggant Development. 37 (5), 527-535. Kenny, M. E., 8 Rice, K. G. (1995). Attachment to parents and adjustment in late adolescent college students: Current status, applications, and future considerations. Counseling Psychologista 23, 433-456. Kerns, K. A., Klepac, L., & Cole, A. K. (1996). Peer relationships and preadolescents’ perceptions of security in mother-child relationship. Developmental Psycholqu, 32, 457-466. Klasner, L. & Pistole, M. C. (2003). College adjustment in a multiethnic sample: attachment, separation-individuation, and ethnic identity. Journal of Collage Stuident Develgpment. 44 (1). 92-109. Kruk, E. (1993). Divorce and disengagement: Patterns of fatherhood within and beyond marriage. Halifax, NS: Femwood. Kurkowski, K., Gordon, D. A., & arbuthnot, J. (1993). Children caught in the middle: A brief educational intervention for parents. Journal of Divorce and Remarriage, 10, 139—151. 193 Lapsley, D. K., Rice, K. G., & Shadid, G. E. (1989). Psychological separation and adjustment to college. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 36, 286-294. Larose, S., & Boivin, M. (1998). Attachment to parents, social support expectations, and socioemotional adjustment during the high school-college transition. Journal of Research on Adolescence 8 1-27. Larson,R. W., Richards, M. H., Moneta, G., Holmbeck, G., & Duckett, E. (1996) Changes in adolescents’ daily interactions with their families from ages 10 to 18: Disengagement and transformation. QavelgqnentiPsvcholggy, 32, 744- 754. Lawson, E. & Thompson, A. (1994). Historical and social conelates of African American divorce: Review of the literature and implications for research. The Western Journal of Black Studies, 18 (2), 91-103. Lehr, R. 81 MacMillan, P. (2001). The psychological and emotional impact of divorce: the noncustodial fathers' perspective. F_amilies in §ocietv. 82 (4), 373- 82. Leonardi, A., & Kiosseoglou, G. (2000). The relationship of parental attachment and psychological separation to the psychological functioning of young adults. Journal of Social Psycholpgy, 140, 418-436. Le Vrne, R. A. & Miller, P. M. (1990). Commentary. _l-leangevelopment, 1%) 73'80. Lewis, M., Feiring, C., & Rosenthal, S. (2000). Attachment over time. Child Development, 71 (3), 707-720. Lindahl, K. M. & Malik, N. M. (1999). Marital conflict, family processes, and boys’ externalizing behavior in Hispanic American and European American families. Journal of (Enical Child Psvcholggy, 28, 12-24. Little, M. A. (1992). The impact of the custody plan on the family: A five year follow-up — executive summary. Family and Conciliation Courts Review, 30, 243-251. Lopez, F. G. (1996). Attachment-related predictors of constructive thinking among college students. Journal of Counseling & Development, 75, 58- 63. Lopez, F. G., Melendez, M. C., & Rice, K. G. (2000). Parental divorce, parent-child bonds, and adult attachment orientations among college students: A 194 comparison of three racial/ethnic groups. Journal of CounseLng Psvcholgv. 47 (2), 177-186. Lytton, H. (1990). Child and parent effects in boys' conduct disorder: A reinterpretation. Developmental Psycholagy, 26, 683-697. Main, M., & Weston, D. R. (1981). The quality of the toddlers’ relationship to mother and to father: Related to conflict behavior and the readiness to establish new relationships. Child Develapment, 52, 932-940. Mason, L. K. (2005). Attachment classification, parental bonding and parental authority as they relate to anxiety, depression, and self-efficacy from a multicultural perspective, Qisserta_ti<_>n Abstract International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering 65 (7Q), 3715. Mathijssen, J. J., Koot, H. M., Verhulst, F. C., DeBruyn, E. J., & Oud, J. H. (1998). The relationship between mutual family relations and child psychopathology. Journal of Child Psycholggy and Psychiat_ry, 39 (4), 477-487. Mattanah, J. F., Hancock, G. R., Brand, B. L. (2004). Parental attachment, separation-individuation, and college student adjustment: A structural equation analysis of mediational effects. Journal of Counseling Psvchologv. 51 (2), 213-225. McCombs, A., & Forehand, R. (1989). Adolescent school performance following parental divorce: Are there family factors that can enhance success? Adolescence, 24, 871-880. McCormick, C. B., & Kennedy, J. H. (2000). Father-child separation, retrospective and current views of attachment relationship with father and self- esteem in late adolescence. Psycholggical Remus, 86, 827-834. McCormick, C. B., & Kennedy, J. H. (1994). Parent-child attachment working models and self-esteem in adolescence. Journal of Youth and Adolescence 23, 1-18. McCurdy, S. J. & Scherman, A. (1996). Effects of family structure on the adolescent separation-individuation process. lilolescence. 31 (12_2), 307-319. McKenry, R, Price, 8., Fine, M., & Servovich, J. (1992). Predictors of single, noncustodial fathers' physical involvement with their children. The Journal of Genetic Psychology. 1%, 305-319. Menaghan, E. & Lieberman, M. (1986). Changes in depression following divorce: A panel study. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 48, 319-328. 195 Meyer, S. A. (1998). Personality correlates of adult attachment. Journal of Social Psycholagy, 138, 407-409. Miller, J. B. & Stubblefield, A. (1993). Parental disclosure from the perspective of late adolescents. Journal of Adolescence, 16 (4), 439-455. Mitchell, A. (1985). Children in the middle. London: Tavistock. Moos, R. H. (1974). Eamilygnvironment Scale. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press. Myers, J. (1992) Wellness, prevention, development: The cornerstone of the profession. Journal of Counseling and Development, 71, 136-139. National Center for Health Statistics (2005). Monthly Vital Statistics Report, 47 (1 ). US. Department of Health and Human Services: Hyattsville, MD Ogbu, J. U. (1988). Cultural diversity and human development. New Directions in Child Development, 42, 11-28. Orbuch, T. L., Thornton, A., & Cancio, J. (2000). The impact of marital quality, divorce, and remarriage on the relationships between parents and their children. Marriage and Family Review. 29 (4). 221-246. Papini, D. R., & Roggman, L. A. (1992). Adolescent perceived attachment to parents in relation to competence, depression, and anxiety: A longitudinal study. Journal of Higher Education, 57, 420-444. Phinney, J. S. (1992). The multigroup ethnic identity measure. Journal of Adolescent ResearChJ, 156-176. Powers, S. l., Hauser, S. T., 8. Kilner, L. A. (1989). Adolescent mental health. American Psychologist. 44 (a), 200-208. Raymond, J. (2001). The ex-files, American Demggraphics, 23, (2), 60—64. Reker, G. T. (1992). Mangal of the Life Attituge Profile-Revised. Peterborough, ON: Student Psychologists Press. Reker, G.T. & Peacock, E. J. (1981). The Life Attitude Profile-R (LAP-R): A multidimensional instrument for assessing attitudes toward life. Canadian Journal of Behavioral Scienceg13. 264-273. Reynolds, W. M. (1982). Development of reliable and valid short forms of the Marlowe-Crowne social desirability scale. Journal of Clinical Psycholggy, 38, 1 19-125. 196 Rice, K. G., & Cummins, P. N. (1996). Late adolescent and parent perceptions of attachment: An exploratory study of personal and social well- being. Journal of Counseling and Development, 75, 50—57. Rice, K. G., Cunningham, T. J., Young, M. B. (1997). Attachment to parents, social competence, and emotional well-being: A comparison of black and white late adolescents. Journal of Counseling Psvchologva44, 89-101. Rice, K. G., Fitzgerald, D. P., Whaley, T. J., & Gibbs, C. L. (1995). Cross- sectional and longitudinal examination of attachment, separation-individuation, and college student adjustment. Journal of Counseling & Development, 73, 463- 474. Rothbaum, F. & Weisz, J. R. (1994). Parental caregiving and child externalizing behavior in nonclinical samples: A meta-analysis. Psycholpgical Bulletin 116, 55-74. Rothbaum, F., Weisz, J., Pott, M., Mijake, K., & Morelli, G. (2000). Attachment and culture: Security in the United States and Japan. American Psychologist, 55, 1093-1 104. Ryan, R. M., & Lynch, J. H. (1989). Emotional autonomy versus detachment: Revisiting the vicissitudes of adolescence and young adulthood. Child Development, 60, 340-356. Sax, L., Astin, A., Korn, W., & Mahoney, K. (1999). The American freshman: National norms for 1999. Los Angeles: Higher Education Research Institute, University of California. Schlack, H. M. (2003). Attachment style and health behaviors. Dissertation Abstract lntemational: Section E he Sciences and Engineering, 63 (8-B), 3971. Schultheiss, D., & Blustein, D. (1994). Role of adolescent-parent relationships in college student development and adjustment. Journal of Counseling Psvchologv. 41, 248-255. Schwarz, J. C. (1999). Schwarz Inter-Parental Conflict Scale (IPC). (abstract) in J. Touliatos, B. P. & Straus, M. (Eds), Handbook of family measurement techniques, Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications, Inc. 485- 486. Scott, D. J. (1999). The effect of parental divorce on the career development process of young adults: A test of separation/attachment theory. Qi_sserta1ign Abstracts International Section B: The Sciences and Engineering. 60 (4-B), 1989. 197 Seaward, B. L. (1995) Reflections on human spirituality for the worksite, American Journal of Health Promotion 9, 165-168. Selzer, J. (1998). Father by law: effects of joint legal custody on nonresident fathers' involvement with children. Demggraphy, 35, 135-146. Seltzer, J. (1994). Consequences of Marital Dissolution for Children. Annual Review of Sociolggy 20, 235-66. Seltzer, J. A., Schaeffer, N. C., & Chamg, H. (1989). Family ties after divorce: The relationship between visiting and paying child support. Journal of Marriage and Family, 51, 1013-1032. Shaw, D. S., Winslow, E. B., & Flanagan, C. (1999). A prospective study of the effects of marital status and family relations on young children’s adjustment among African American and European American families. Child Development, 70(3), 742-755. Stevenson, M. R., & Black, K. N. (1995). How (fivorce affects offspring: A research approach. Madison, WI: Brown and Benchmark. Stone, G., & McKenry, P. C. ( 1998). Nonresidential father involvement: a test of a mid-range theory. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 159 (3), 313-36. Summers, P., Forehand, R., Annistead, L., & Tannenbaum, L. (1998). Parental divorce during early adolescence in Caucasian families: The role of family process variables in predicting the long-term consequences for early adult psychosocial adjustment. Journal of COtglseling and Clinical Psycholggy, 66 (2), 327-336. Tate, R. L. (1998). An introduction to modeling outcomes in the behavioral and social sciences (2nd ed.) Edina, MN: Burgess Publishing. Taub, D. J. (1995). Relationship of selected factors to traditional-age undergraduate women’s development of autonomy. Journal of Collage Student Developmellt, 36(2), 141-151. Thompson, R.A, Lamb, M., & Estes, D. (1982). Stability of infant-mother attachment and its relationship to changing life circumstances in an unselected middle-class sample. Child Development, 53, 144-148. Travis J. W., Callander M. G. (1990). Wellness for Helping Professionals. Mill Valley, CA: Wellness Associates. 198 US. Census Bureau. (2005). US. report on census population, 2004. Washington, DC: Government Printing Office. US. Census Bureau. (2003). US. remrt on census population, 2002. Washington, DC: Government Printing Office. US. Census Bureau. (2001). US. remit on census population, 1996. Washington, DC: Government Printing Office. Van Ijzendoom, M. H., & DeWolff, M. S. (1997). In search of the absent father — meta-analyses of infant-father attachment: A rejoinder to our discussants. Child Development. 68 (4). 604-609. Van IJzendoom, M. H., & Kroonenberg, P. M. (1988). Cross-cultural patterns of attachment: A meta-analysis of the strange situation. Child Development, 59, 147-156. Van IJzendoom, M. H., & Sagi, A. (1999). Cross-cultural patterns of attachment. In J. Cassidy & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of Attachment (713- 734). New York: Guilford. Vaughn, 3., Egeland, B., Sroufe, L. A., & Waters, E. (1997). Individual differences in infant-mother attachment at twelve and eighteen months: Stability and change in families under stress. Child Development, 50, 971-975. Veneziano, R. A. & Rohner, R. P. (1998). Perceived patemai acceptance, patemai involvement, and youths” psychological adjustment in a rural, biracial southern community. Journal of Marriage and the Famiy, 60, 335-343. Vivona, J. (2000). Parental attachment styles of late adolescents: Qualities of attachment relationships and consequences for adjustment. Journal of Qaunseling Psvcholm 47. 316-329. Wallerstein, J.S.. & Kelly, J.B. (1980). Surviving the break-up: How children and parents cog with t_livorca New York: Basic Books. Weiss, R. (1975). Marital Separation. New York: Basic Books. Whiteside, M. F. & Becker, B. J. (2000). Parental factors and the young child’s postdivorce adjustment: A meta-analysis with implications for parenting arrangements. Journal of Family Psvcholggy, 14, 5—26. Whiteside, M. F. (1998). The parental alliance following divorce: Literature review and policy implications. Journal of Marital and Famiu Therapy, 24, 3-24. 199 Whitmer, J. M. & Sweeney, T. J. (1992). A holistic model for wellness prevention over the life span. Journal of Counseling and Development, 71, 140- 148. Woodward, L., Pergusson, D. M., & Belsky, J. (2000). Timing of parental separation and attachment to parents in adolescence: Results of a prospective study from birth to age 16. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 62, 162-174. Zayas, L. H. 81 Solari, F. (1994). Early childhood socialization in Hispanic families: Context, culture, and practice implications. Professional Psychology; Research and Practice, 25(3), 200-206. Zayas, L. H., & Palleja, J. (1988). Puerto Rican familism: Implications for family therapy. Family Relations, 37, 260-264. Zimmerman, M., Salem, 0., & Maton, K. (1995) Family structure and psychosocial correlates among urban African-American adolescent males. Child Develmment, 66, 1 598-161 . 200 I);illjiirjjjijjgii