Nuwwwfi “E... affifirnmia. 1...]; . 3? fizfiéfii 2:: :1. .06.”. . v 3:! .1 z a. ) tls . . ‘ 191.94.!” .11: ; 3.3x €3.21) ~ A. Riv okra? at... 12.»... I! .V 3...}...131‘ .91....‘2‘134. A7011: 3.39:... . zv :4331 .34.. xi. “his.“ .. . p x 1...; » .141)? 0.3.2. _ 2 3:: .29“? 2:... 1):. a. 1.. .stl... 73(34- .3 a . V!- ‘vs 1 o 350.93.11.70: 1 .q . £19: 23%.. . uh... is. ) HF 5.127. - {.31 . IV hat: a .. a .53 , T . {P1, . 1...}... aux-1H! ,3...,1m:-_ . :1. a. i. 31““. O ....I x .. z... . .x... . .n .. mfl-wmmr-zngngfingu $1 53%;“... .n, gamma“ wag-13.5w .. .u. . 313...... z:- . :2. ._ . .....- . at; 7‘ 5 WHEN“ i. . - — uty V310: ~.N!u-‘Hfl..w.fl..: S . . &§_r§r:a-m¥£§ . .6: 2143.... 4. 1! t‘.. ‘- LIBRARY Michigan State University This is to certify that the timis entitled AUDITORY SYSTEM FUNCTION IN INDIVIDUALS WITH WILLIAMS SYNDROME pmenbd by YAO—JAN LIU, B.S. tabeenacoepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for the M.A. degree in DEPARTMENT OF AUDIOLOGY AND SPEECH SCIENCES V Major Profaeor‘s Signature LEI/15%? Date MSU is an Affirmative Action/Equal Opportunity Institution PLACE IN RETURN Box to remove this checkout from your record. To AVOID FINES return on or before date due. MAY BE RECAUED with earlier due date if requested. DATE DUE DATE DUE DATE DUE 2/05 c:/CI'RC/oateoue.ind¢' 'p' .15‘ AUDITORY SYSTEM FUNCTION IN INDIVIDUALS WITH WILLIAMS SYNDROME BY Yao-Jan Liu, 8.8. A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF ARTS Department of Audiology and Speech Sciences 2005 ABSTRACT AUDITORY SYSTEM FUNCTION IN INDIVIDUALS WITH WILLIAMS SYNDROME By Yao-Jan Liu, B.S. Hypersensitivity to sounds is a well-known characteristic of individuals with Williams syndrome (WS); however, auditory system function in individuals with WS has not yet been thoroughly evaluated. The purpose of this research project was to test objectively whether individuals with WS are more sensitive to auditory stimulation (have better-than-normal thresholds). Seven individuals with WS, between the ages of 7 and 43 years, were recruited for the current study. Audiological and physiological status was examined using air- and bone-conduction thresholds, tympanometry, loudness discomfort level measures, distortion-product otoacoustic emissions (DPOAEs) and a hyperacusis questionnaire that was adapted from Klein, Armstrong, Greer, and Brown (1990). The results of this study revealed that five of the seven individuals with WS showed pure-tone thresholds poorer than expected when compared to age peers from the general population. Only one of them had a documented diagnosis of permanent hearing loss before this study. The empirical evidence presented here not only suggests that reports of hypersensitivity to sounds do not justify a conclusion that overall hearing is intact or better than normal in individuals with WS, but also addresses the importance of early diagnosis of hearing loss in this population. Copyright by YAO-J AN LIU 2005 ACKNOWLEDGMENTS This study was funded by a grant awarded to thesis co-director, Jeffery Marler (NICHD R03 HD044468). I would like to express my sincere thanks to the co-chairs on my thesis committee, Dr. Jeffrey Marler and Dr. Jill Elfenbein, for all their support and their invaluable comments on my drafts. I would also like to thank the other committee members, Dr. Jerry Punch and Dr. Rachel Fisher. Special gratitude is extended to all the families who participated in this study, to the support shown by the National Director of the Williams Syndrome Association, Terry Monkaba, and to Dr. Carolyn Meris for assistance with subject recruitment. iv TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES .................................................................................. vii LIST OF FIGURES ................................................................................ viii CHAPT ER 1 Introduction ............................................................................................ 1 Literature review ...................................................................................... 3 Behavioral assessment of hearing .............................................................. 3 Hyperacusis and otitis media ................................................................... 5 Auditory verbal strengths ........................................................................ 8 Audiological evaluation of individuals with cognitive impairment ..................... 10 Summary ......................................................................................... l4 Rationale for current study ..................................................................... 14 CHAPTER 2 Method ................................................................................................ 16 Participants.................................‘ ...................................................... 16 Procedures ....................................................................................... 17 Pure-tone thresholds ........................................................................ l7 Otoscopy and tympanometry ............................................................. 19 Speech audiometry ......................................................................... 19 Otoacoustic emissions data ................................................................ 20 Loudness discomfort level (LDL) rating ................................................ 21 Hyperacusis questionnaire ................................................................ 23 CHAPTER 3 Results ................................................................................................ 24 Audiological assessment ....................................................................... 24 Otoscopy ......................................................................................... 24 Immitance ........................................................................................ 24 Pure-tone audiometry .......................................................................... 24 Speech audiometry32 Otoacoustic emission data ..................................................................... 34 Test—retest reliability ........................................................................... 38 Loudness discomfort level (LDL) rating .................................................... 41 Hyperacusis questionnaire ..................................................................... 42 Hypersensitivity to sounds .................................................................... 42 Hearing ........................................................................................ 44 Middle ear infection ........................................................................ 44 CHAPTER 4 Discussion ............................................................................................ 45 Audiological assessment ....................................................................... 45 Otoacoustic emission data ..................................................................... 53 Loudness discomfort level (LDL) rating .................................................... 54 Hyperacusis questionnaire ..................................................................... 55 Evaluation procedures .......................................................................... 57 Summary and conclusion ...................................................................... 58 APPENDICES ....................................................................................... 60 APPENDIX A Hyperacusis Questionnaire .................................................................... 61 APPENDIX B Individual Data from Immitance Measues ................................................... 65 REFERENCES ....................................................................................... 67 vi LIST OF TABLES Table 1. Gender and age data for each participant (M: Male; F: Female). .................. 17 Table 2. Summary of quantitative measures administrated to participants in the current study. ................................................................................................. 17 Table 3. Data comparison of extended high-frequency air-conduction thresholds obtained from WSOO4 and mean thresholds for 32 individuals age 20 to 30 years reported by Stelmachowicz et al. (1989). ...................................................................... 32 Table 4. Speech audiometry and pure-tone-average data for the four adults who completed the speech audiometry tasks. 33 Table 5. Participant WSOlS’s air-conduction thresholds obtained from hearing screening and diagnostic evaluation. ......................................................................... 39 Table 6. Loudness discomfort level rating scale data for the adult participants. ........... 41 Table 7. Individual data from immitance measures. ........................................... 66 vii LIST OF FIGURES Figure 1. Picture provided for LDL rating measure ............................................ 22 Figure 2. Hearing thresholds for each participant (0: Right ear air-conduction thresholds; X: Left ear air-conduction thresholds; <: Unmasked right ear bone-conduction thresholds; >: Unmasked left bone-conduction thresholds; [: Masked right ear bone-conduction thresholds; ]: Masked left ear bone-conduction thresholds). .................................. 25 Figure 3. Individual DPOAEs for participants WSOO], WSOO2, WSOO3, WSOO4, and WSOlS. ............................................................................................... 35 Figure 4. DPOAEs of participant WSOIS obtained from hearing screening and diagnostic evaluation. ........................................................................................... 40 Figure 5. Comparison of the four adult participants’ air-conduction thresholds to expected median thresholds. The median was calculated from data and formulas provided by ISO 7029-1984 according to each individual’s age and gender (0: Right ear air-conduction thresholds; X: Left ear air—conduction thresholds; *: Median threshold data calculated from ISO 7029-1984). .............................................................................. 47 Figure 6. Comparison of the three child participants’ air-conduction thresholds to the median thresholds reported by Holmes et a1. (1994). The median was selected according to each participant’s age and gender (0: Right ear air-conduction thresholds; X: Left ear air-conduction thresholds; *: Median threshold data reported by Holmes et al.). ..........50 viii CHAPTER 1 Introduction Williams syndrome (WS), also known as Williams-Beuren syndrome, was reported by Fanconi (1952), and Williams, Barratt-Boyes, & Lowe (1961). Typical features of Williams syndrome include specific facial dysmorphology (“elfm” features), renal and cardiovascular anomalies, failure to thrive in infancy, transient—neonatal hypercalcemia, elastin arteriopathy, mild-to-moderate mental retardation or learning difficulties, delayed language and motor development, a unique cognitive profile, a distinctive personality, and hypersensitivity to speech and abnormal (aversive) sensitivities to other classes of sounds (Mom's, Demsey, Leonard, Dilts, & Blackburn, 1988; Lenhoff, Wang, Greenberg, & Bellugi, 1997; Bellugi, Lichtenberger, Jones, & Lai, 2000; Morris & Mervis, 2000). Historically, the incidence of WS has been reported to be at least l/20,000 individuals (Morris et al., 1988). Recently, a Norwegian population prevalence study has estimated a prevalence closer to 1/7,500 live births (Morris, 2004). The current understanding of genetic causes of WS involves a micro-deletion of the long arm of chromosome 7 (Ewart et al. 1993; Korenberg et al., 2000). This micro-deletion includes gene-encoding elastin (ELN) and 17-20 other genes (Bellugi et al., 2000; DeSilva etal., 2002; Ewart et al., 1993; Merla, Ucla, Guipponi, & Reymond, 2002; Osborne, 1999) and can be detected by fluorescent in situ hybridization (FISH) in 99% of individuals with WS (Lowery et al., 1995). Relative to their overall level of intellectual ability, individuals with WS typically show strengths in the areas of phonology (Grant et al., 1997; Volterra, Capirci & Caselli, 2001), auditory (verbal) short-term memory (Grant et al., 1997, Mervis & Klein-Tasman, 2000), and expressive vocabulary (Mervis & Robinson, 2000; Singer-Harris, Bellugi & Bates, 1997). The relative strengths in phonology and vocabulary have been attributed to auditory processing strengths (Karmiloff-Smith et al., 1997; Mervis & Klein-Tasman, 2000). Both auditory verbal strengths (Karmiloff-Smith et al., 1997) and auditory language comprehension abilities (Neville, Mills, & Bellugi, 1994) of individuals with WS have been attributed to hypersensitivity to sound. When data collection for this project was begun, these inferences were supported solely by anecdotal reports of auditory processing strengths and surprising musical abilities (Lenhoff et al., 1997; Mervis & Klein-Tasman, 2000) and brief papers and questionnaire-studies reporting high rates of extreme oversensitivity to sound (Van Borsel, Curfs, & Fryns, 1997; Klein, Armstrong, Greer and Brown, 1990; O’Reilly, Lacey and Lancioni, 2000; Johnson, Comeau & Clarke, 2001). Two descriptive studies recounting audiometric performances of a small number of individuals with WS (Ruangdaraganon, Tocharoentanaphol, Kotchabhakdi, & Khowsathit, 1999; Johnson et al., 2001) indicated that 33% had sensorineural hearing loss. These two recent reports of significant rates of hearing loss in WS are difficult to reconcile with the popular understanding of superior auditory sensitivity. The literature available provides only a limited foundation for exploring this inconsistency. Literature Review Behavioral assessment of hearing Audiometric performance in individuals with WS, or those suspected of having WS (i.e., Nigam & Samuel, 1994), has been reported in three descriptive studies (Nigam & Samuel, 1994; Ruangdaraganon et al., 1999; Johnson et al., 2001). Nigam and Samuel (1994) reported a case study of a 21-month-old girl with WS in Sunderland, Britain. She was seen by a geneticist, and a diagnosis of WS was based on her facial characteristics rather than on a FISH confirmation. She was referred to an ENT clinic because of intermittent otalgia, drooling and snoring accompanied by a clear nasal discharge. The authors reported observation of dull tympanic membranes and collapsed nasal alae, and that, “on a performance test a 35-40 dB loss was found on the left side and impedance testing gave a flat trace on the left but normal responses on the right” (p. 494). They also reported that, six months after adenoidectomy and bilateral myringotomies, her response to everyday sounds, including the ringing of a doorbell or telephone, was to clasp her hands over her ears. A follow-up audiologic assessment revealed hearing within normal limits for both ears. Ruangdaraganon et al. (1999) reported a case study of two individuals with WS who were identified by the FISH technique. Participants were a 15-month-old girl and a 16-month-old girl seen in Bangkok, Thailand. Ruangdaraganon et al. reported that one girl had auditory brainstem responses (ABR) indicative of a bilateral sensorineural hearing loss. However, they did not report the severity of the hearing loss or the status of the middle ear function. They did not include otoacoustic emissions measures (OAE) in their test battery, although a combination of ABR and OAE is commonly used for early identification of hearing loss. Such data would have provided confirmation of the ABR results and important information about the site of lesion. For the other participant, they reported no information concerning hearing assessment or her hearing status. Johnson et al. (2001) contacted 23 families from the Atlantic Provinces of Canada. Nine individuals with WS, ages 9 to 25 years, from these families underwent a full battery of audiologic tests. Their test battery included pure—tone audiometry, speech recognition thresholds, acoustic reflex thresholds, tympanometry, and transient evoked otoacoustic emission (TEOAE) measures. Researchers found that sensorineural hearing loss was present in three participants ages 18, 24, and 25 years within this group. All three showed moderate sensorineural hearing loss; one had bilateral hearing loss and two showed unilateral hearing loss. Seven of the nine participants, including these three participants with sensorineural hearing loss and four participants with normal pure-tone thresholds, were reported to have absent TEOAEs. Johnson et al. (2001) failed to define what constituted “normal” hearing thresholds for a group of adults with mild-to-moderate mental retardation, did not report speech recognition thresholds, neglected to quantify the magnitude terminology (e. g., “moderate”), and did not describe the configuration of the hearing thresholds. Furthermore, they did not include the pass/fail criterion for the OAE measures. Absent TEOAEs suggested abnormal outer hair cell function. The absence of TEOAEs in the individuals with normal hearing (4 of 6 subjects) suggests a lack of sensitivity in the measurement of hearing loss. Marler, Elfenbein, Netzloff & Liu (2004) investigated hearing acuity in 21 individuals with WS at the 2004 National Williams Syndrome Conference in Grand Rapids, Michigan. Fourteen children, ages 7 to 9 years, and seven adults, ages 21 to 48 years, participated in their preliminary study. The test battery included pure-tone, air- conduction screening at the frequencies of 500, 1,000, 2,000, 4,000, and 8,000 Hz, screening tympanometry, and DPOAE measures. The criterion for failure of the pure tone hearing screening was failure to respond to a 20 dB HL signal at any target frequency in either ear for the adults, and failure to respond to a 15 dB HL signal at any target frequency in either ear for the children. The criterion for a DPOAE indicative of hearing loss was an emission below the 5th percentile point for normally hearing ears reported by Gorga et a1. (1997). One of the adult participants was not included in the analyses because a second genetic condition (Waardenburg syndrome) was later confirmed. Sixteen of 20 individuals (80%) with WS in that study showed normal bilateral tympanograms (Type A). Two more individuals had normal unilateral tympanograms, but tympanograms could not be obtained on their other ears. No tympanograms were obtained for two other individuals due to excessive cerumen or complaint of ear pain. Marler et al. reported that 84% of their participants failed the hearing screening and 74% showed DPOAEs indicative of impaired cochlear function. Hyperacusis and otitis media J astreboff & Jastreboff (2000) defined hyperacusis as “abnormally strong reactions occurring within the auditory pathways resulting from exposure to even a moderate sound level” (p. 156). They defined “phonophobia” as “abnormally strong reactions of autonomic and limbic systems without abnormally high activation of auditory system resulting from enhanced connections between the auditory and limbic system” (p. 156). They noted that individuals with phonophobia have developed fear of sound as the result of their hyperacusis. Dix, Hallpike, and Hood (1948) reported that patients with sensorineural hearing loss who complain of discomfort from loud sounds are diagnosed as having “loudness recruitment.” Loudness recruitment has been defined as “an abnormally rapid growth of loudness” (p.219) in individuals with cochlear impairment (Mendel, Danhauer, & Singh, 1999) and is a phenomenon similar to the hypersensitivity to loud sounds seen in hyperacusis. The commonality between hyperacusis and auditory recruitment is intolerance of loud sounds. The phenomenon of hyperacusis has been reported in greater than 95% of the individuals with WS (Klein et al., 1990; Nigam & Samuel, 1994; Van Borsel et al., 1997). Klein et a1. investigated the prevalence of hyperacusis and otitis media in individuals with WS by conducting a survey. They noted that earlier attempts to obtain audiometric data from individuals with WS had been unsuccessful because of factors related to age, hyperactivity, and/or developmental age. Thus, they used a survey approach rather than a complete audiological assessment (i.e., threshold audiogram, high- level speech discrimination, and acoustic reflex measures). Participants for the survey were recruited from a Williams Syndrome Association membership list of four southeastern states. A questionnaire was sent to 100 parents of individuals with WS. Sixty-five surveys (65%), covering 36 male and 29 female individuals ages 1 to 28 years, were completed and returned. Children attending a pediatric, general dentistry clinic served as a normal control group. The control group included 32 males and 33 females between the ages of 2 to 17 years. According to parental report, prevalence for hyperacusis was 95% in the individuals with WS and was 12% in the control group. Specific information about offending sounds and the reactions to these sounds were also reported in this study. Firecracker, power saw, electronic drill, fire engine siren, motorcycle, loud auto muffler, and blender were each selected by more than 60% of the parents in this survey. Thunder, the most common write-in answer, was reported by 19% of parents. Sounds that are not usually associated with threatening events such as automatic icemaker, television test tone, newspaper crackling, and church bells were added to the list by some parents. Loud (73%) and sudden (74%) were the descriptive characteristics of offending sounds most frequently selected by parents. In the Klein et a1. study, the prevalence of otitis media in the WS population (61%) was significantly greater than the prevalence in the normal group (30%). The Spearman rank-order correlation coefficient was used to investigate the relationship between frequency of otitis media and the overall hyperacusis rating. A significant relationship could not be established (r = .156, p < .10) in that study. The authors reported that, among individuals with WS, boys tend to have more episodes of otitis media than girls, and girls tend to have the placement of tympanostomy tubes slightly more often. In the case study presented by Nigam and Samuel (1994), a left middle ear pathology, left conductive hearing loss and the phenomenon of hyperacusis were reported in a 21-month-old child. They described the subject’s responses to sounds as a result of lack of exposure to environmental sounds due to temporary conductive hearing loss secondary to middle ear pathology. Nigam and Samuel reviewed the previous WS research, but failed to find support for their lack-of-exposure hypothesis. Van Borsel et al. (1997) obtained survey data on hyperacusis and otitis media in individuals with WS. Their questionnaire was similar to the questionnaire used by Klein et al. (1990). The survey was mailed to all parents of children in the Dutch and the Flemish WS associations. The number distributed was not reported. Eighty-two usable questionnaires were returned from the parents. The ages of the individuals with WS ranged from 1 year 10 months to 45 years 5 months. Only 4 (5%) out of 82 subjects had “never been frightened or bothered by sounds” (p. 128). The majority of the participants (83%) reported “oversensitivity” to sounds and the remaining participants (12%) reported that they had experienced this problem in the past. Parental report showed that individuals in that 12% had outgrown their fear of sounds at ages ranging from several months to 22 years. The authors did not differentiate hyperacusis and phonophobia in their subjects. Fifty-four percent of the WS subjects in the Van Borsel et al. study noted above were reported to have had episodes of otitis media, and 76% were reported to show behavior indicative of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Van Borsel et al. reported that the tendency for boys to have more episodes of otitis media and girls to have more placements of tympanostomy tubes, reported by Klein et al. (1994), could not be observed in their study. The Spearman rank-order correlation coefficient was used to investigate the relation between frequency of otitis media and the overall hyperacusis rating, and no significant relation could be established (r = -.l285, p = .253). Auditory verbal strengths Individuals with WS show a relative strength in verbal short-term memory (Mervis etal., 2000; wain & Yule, 1991). This ability has been hypothesized to provide an important foundation for later vocabulary acquisition (Mervis, Robinson, Rowe, Becerra, & Klein-Tasman, 2003). Even when demonstrating mild-to-moderate mental retardation, children with WS will frequently score within 1 standard deviation of the general population on standardized measures of auditory rote recall and working memory (Mervis et al., 2003). Neville et a1. (1994) investigated the impact of auditory sensitivity on the development of individuals with WS using the event-related potential (ERP) technique. Four children (two males and two females) ages 10 to 14 years and four adults (one male and three females) served as subjects for their study. One of the adults was tested both as a child at the age of 14 and as an adult at the age of 18. They also recruited normal control children and adults. Approximately half of the control subjects were male. The paradigm of Auditory Recovery Cycle was designed to study the early stage of auditory sensory processing and the neural basis for the hypersensitivity to sounds that individuals with WS showed. One thousand and 1,500 Hz tonal stimuli were presented through earphones. Ninety percent of the stimuli were 1,500 Hz tones, and the others were 1,000 Hz tones. The subjects were asked to respond the target tones (1,000 Hz) by pressing a button. The second part of the research was to investigate the early and late neural systems that may be important to auditory language processing in individuals with WS and also to compare the organization of neural systems important to reading in those who learned to read earlier versus later. In separate conditions, subjects listened to or read sentences and then judged whether the sentences made sense. Sample sentences provided by authors included, “After the snow they clapped their hands” and “I don’t go to school on Saturday or floor” (p. 71). ERP measures were recorded during both the tone and the sentence tasks. The results showed that tone-elicited ERPs from the individuals with WS showed a normal morphology; however, they were considerably less refractory than tone—elicited ERPs of the normal controls. That is, a short interstimulus interval reduced the amplitude of response in the normal controls but not in the individuals with WS. During the sentence task, the ERPs elicited with visually presented words were the same for the control subjects and the individuals with WS. However, the morphology of ERPs elicited during listening tasks in individuals with WS were described as highly abnormal and characterized by a greater positive/enhanced response to related lexical items. The pattern of results in this study suggested that the organization and operation of cognitive systems could be influenced by alterations in early sensory processing. Neville et a1. concluded that the ERP evidence of hypersensitivity in the auditory systems of individuals with WS may provide important clues to the processes underlying the precocious nature of language skill development in these individuals. They also noted that the ERP data provide insight into differences in auditory and visual processing by individuals with WS. Audiological evaluation of individuals with cognitive impairment Individuals with WS and individuals with Down syndrome (DS) demonstrate an equivalent cognitive impairment (Bellugi, Wang, & Jemigan, 1994) and a high prevalence of otitis media (Balkany, Downs, & Jafek, & Krajicek, 1979; Klein et al., 1990; Van Bosel et al., 1997). Research in developing hearing assessment strategies for use with individuals with WS has not yet been initiated. As was discussed earlier, researchers have reported difficulties in conducting audiological assessment in 10 individuals with WS (Klein, 1994). Therefore, data concerning individuals with D8 will be used as a foundation for determining procedures to be used with individuals with WS. Kile (1996) provides a summary of the types of modifications that can be made to standard assessment procedures so that the procedures are appropriate given the behavioral characteristics of children with DS (e. g., social responsiveness, tendency to produce echolalic imitations). He reported that children with DS usually show compliant, interactive behavior during audiologic assessment, and that a wise use of social and tactile interaction and reinforcement often keeps them focused and increases time available to obtain reliable results. Children with DS tend to make more echolalic imitations than do normally developing children (Rondal, Lambert, & Sohier, 1981); therefore, Kile suggested that teaching children with DS to mimic auditory stimuli can increase consistency of response when conventional behavioral testing procedures are not successful. Kile also reported success using this modification in a l2-year-old child with DS who was seen in his clinic. This child could not be conditioned to do play audiometry and did not show consistent responses in VRA; however, he responded to pure tones by saying “Beep Beep.” Kile noted that pure-tone threshold measures were obtained using this modification. Kile conducted the ear canal volume measures for 10 children with DS and 10 children with normal development, and reported that children with DS showed a smaller ear canal volume than normally developing children. He recommended that the establishment of the ear canal volume measures of children with DS can be helpful when comparing pre- and post-tympanometry tube placement measurements. In order to investigate the prevalence of otitis media in children with DS, Kile evaluated 47 children 11 (mean age = 3.6 years) with DS in his clinic. He reported that 74% of the children with DS in his project demonstrated abnormal tympanograms that were consistent with the presence of otitis media, and that 58% of them had this condition bilaterally. Kile also suggested that otoacoustic emission (OAE) measures may be useful in assessing cochlear function in children with DS. However, OAEs can only provide accurate information when individuals do not have middle ear disease. Kile recommended that, due to the high incidence of middle ear disease and the risk of hearing impairment in children with DS, early identification of hearing loss in children with DS is critical and audiologic assessment should be performed every four to six months once the hearing loss has been diagnosed. Dille (2003) summarized strategies for effective testing, as well as common findings, for individuals with DS across the lifespan. Her report was based on a review of previous research concerning hearing assessments of individuals with DS. Dille reported that behavioral testing procedures such as VRA are successful when the appropriate developmental criterion is used. She noted that VRA procedures provide reliable audiometric information when children with DS have obtained a developmental age of 10 to 12 months, due to delayed sound localization skills in children with DS (Greenburg, Wilson, Moore, & Thompson, 1978). Dille reported “pure-tone testing to be reliable for children with DS only after 8 years of age, whereas objective procedures are more reliable for the younger p0pulation” (p. 204). Dille recommended the use of objective techniques (e.g., ABR, DPOAE) to diagnose hearing loss in adults and children with DS, and she also noted that even when 12 individuals with DS have normal tympanometry and audiometry, their DPOAE measures demonstrated lower amplitude than normal age-peers. Keiser, Montague, Wold, Maune and Pattison (1981) collected complete sets of audiometric information, including air- and bone-conduction thresholds, tympanometry and acoustic reflexes from 51 adults with DS (aged 15.1 to 51.6 years). They obtained pure—tone thresholds using standard pure-tone audiometry (hand-raising response) or conditioned play audiometry from 46 of the 51 participants. They used 15 dB HL as the criterion for identifying hearing loss, and found that 75% of the participants exhibited hearing loss in at least one ear. This study showed a sloping sensorineural hearing loss above 4,000 Hz, which is indicative of early presbycusis, and this configuration became significant by adolescence and progressedthroughout their lifespans. They also reported that tympanograms from 39 % of the tested ears suggested abnormal middle ear function. Their study also indicated that reliable thresholds could be obtained using conventional pure-tone audiometry both for older children (15 years old and older) and for adults with DS. Review of the available literature showed that procedures for the assessment hearing loss in individuals with DS have been well established. Because characteristic similarities between WS and DS have been reported in the literature (Bellugi et al., 1994; Balkany et al., 1979; Klein et al., 1990; Van Bosel et al., 1997), hearing assessments for individuals with DS may be modified for use with individuals with WS. Modified behavioral assessments described by Kile ( 1996) and Dille (2003) provide important information on how to approach behavioral assessment of individuals with WS based on the characteristics of individuals with DS. Once children with WS have developed the 13 skill of sound localization, VRA can be introduced to estimate behavioral thresholds. Modified VRA using earphones can be introduced to obtain behavioral thresholds in young children who are not able to do conditioned play audiometry. Conditioned play audiometry can be used to obtain thresholds in children by age 8 years who are able to demonstrate reliable responses to pure-tone stimuli. For adults, conditioned play audiometry and conventional pure-tone audiometry with verbal or hand-raising response can be used for hearing assessment. Summay In summary, previous research reports from independent labs have indicated a prevalence of sensorineural hearing loss in individuals with WS ranging from 33% (Ruangdaraganon et al., 1999; Johnson et al., 2001) to 74% (Marler et al., 2004). These studies have included small numbers of participants, provided incomplete data about test protocols, failed to define the degree of hearing loss, or have reported auditory information gathered in acoustically uncontrolled environments. As a result, interpretation of these data is confounded and application of the data to a clinical setting is limited. Rationale for the current study The hearing acuity of individuals with WS has not yet been well documented. It is necessary to establish audiometric performance in individuals with WS before the strengths or weaknesses of auditory processing and language abilities in WS can be accurately evaluated. The purpose of this research project was to test objectively whether individuals with WS are more sensitive to auditory stimulation (i.e., have better-than- normal thresholds) than their normally developing age-peers. 14 l) 2) 3) The goal of this project was to answer the following questions: Do individuals with WS have normal hearing? Although the data available regarding prevalence of hearing loss in individuals with WS are incomplete, there is growing evidence that a significant portion of this population demonstrates hearing loss. Therefore, it was hypothesized that the prevalence of hearing loss is greater in individuals with WS than in normally hearing age peers. Is there normal outer hair cell function in the individuals with WS? Although hyperacusis has been seen as a phenomenon similar to loudness recruitment, few studies have used OAE data to investigate the outer cell function of individuals with WS who demonstrated hyperacusis. Johnson et al. (2001) had subjects with normal hearing and absent OAEs; Marler et al. (2004) did not. Neither set of authors presented hyperacusis data for their subjects. It was hypothesized that the outer hair cell function measured by DPOAE in the current study would be consistent with audiometric data. Do individuals with WS report discomfort from sound levels that are below the loudness discomfort level typically reported by individuals with loudness recruitment associated with sensorineural hearing loss? Although hypersensitivity to sounds has been reported in previous studies, information on loudness discomfort levels in individuals with WS is not available. It was hypothesized that individuals with WS will demonstrate lower loudness discomfort levels than individuals with sensorineural hearing loss. 15 CHAPTER 2 Method Participants Seven individuals with WS, between the ages of 7 to 43 years, were recruited through contacts with the Great Lakes Region branch of the National Williams Syndrome Association, special-needs educators, geneticists, and private physicians, and through advertisements placed in local and regional association newsletters. All subjects participated in a broad study of communication skills conducted under the direction of thesis co-advisor Jeffrey Marler. Only the audiological data gathered are reported here. FISH was previously completed on all the participants in the current study. The results were either reviewed by Michael Netzloff, M.D., Director of Clinical Genetics at Michigan State University (MSU) for confirmation of the diagnosis of WS, or were confirmed by previous participation in the longitudinal projects of Carolyn B. Mervis, Ph.D., Director of the Cognition and Language Laboratory at the University of Louisville. In all cases, FISH revealed the absence of one copy of the elastin gene on chromosome 7 and all had clinical phenotype characteristics of WS. Following explanation of the study, a parent or a legal guardian of each participant was asked to read and sign an informed consent form. In addition, all participants were asked to sign an assent form. Consent procedures were approved by the MSU University Committee on Research Involving Human Subjects (UCRIHS IRB #02- 534). Each participant was paid $15 for participating in this study. Due to the low incidence of WS, participants were recruited from a wide geographic area; some of the participants had to travel hundreds of miles and stayed in a 16 hotel the night before attending this study. Thus, it was not possible to recruit each participant for a second visit to assess test-retest variability in performance. Table 1 provides gender and age data for the seven participants in this project. Table 1. Gender antljge data for each participant (M: Male; F: Female). Chronological Age Part1c1pant # Gender (years; months) ws001 M 43;1 ws002 F 392 WSOO3 M 175 WSOO4 M 26:2 wsom M 7:1 WSOl8 F 7:8 wsozs F 1 “8 13mm Table 2 provides a summary of the test battery administrated to each participant. The test battery was modified to meet the individual needs of the participants (i.e., short attention span, difficulties in understanding the task, fatigue, fear of the probes placed in their ears). All the audiometric data were collected by the author under the supervision of an ASHA certified speech-language pathologist and/or an ASHA certified audiologist. Table 2. Summary of quantitative measures administrated to participants in the current study. Measures Independent Variable Dependent Variable Air-conduction thresholds dB HL (.5 to 8 kHz) Auditory sensitivity Bone-conduction thresholds dB HL (.5 to 4 kHz) Auditory sensitivity Tympanometry daPa at tympanic peak Middle ear status Speech recognition threshold (SRT) dB HL Speech perception ability Word recognition score (WRS & % Correct Speech perception ability NU-6 list) DPOAE dB SPL Outer hair cell function (Cochlea integrity) Pure-tone thresholds The goal was to obtain air-conduction thresholds for the frequency range of 500 to 8,000 Hz, and bone—conduction thresholds for the frequency range of 500 to 4,000 Hz 17 at octave intervals. For the first four participants, these measures were made after the language measures that were part of the broader study. After that portion of data collection was completed, it became clear that to reduce the impact of factors such as fatigue and short attention span on the participants’ responses, the audiometric data needed to be collected at the beginning of each participant’s session before the language measures were administered. For this reason, the audiometric data were gathered first for the last three participants. The signals for the standard pure-tone audiometry were pulsed tones generated by a GSI- 16 audiometer and delivered by TDH-49 earphones or mastoid placement of a bone conduction vibrator. Participants were seated in an Industrial Acoustics Corporation test booth during this portion of the study. Adult participants were asked to respond to stimuli by raising a hand, and children performed a play audiometry task in which they put a block in a puzzle board. Thresholds 2 20 dB HL at any frequency in adults and 15 dB HL at any frequency in children were considered to indicate hearing loss. The degrees of hearing impairment were defined using the categories recommended by Jerger and Jerger (1980) for adult participants and by Northern and Downs (2002) for child participants. The criterion for identifying hearing loss as sensorineural was that there be no more than a single air-bone gap greater than 10 dB HL in the pure-tone thresholds. For the individuals for whom the masked bone-conduction thresholds could not be obtained. the results of tympanometry were used to rule out middle ear pathology. One individual in the current study was able to participate in extended high- frequency air-conduction audiometry in addition to the basic audiometric test battery. Extended high-frequency air-conduction audiometry, gathered for 10,000, 12,500, and 18 16,000 Hz stimuli, was conducted using a Madsen Orbiter 922 Version 2 audiometer and Sennheiser HDA200 earphones. Both GSI- 16 and Orbiter 922 Version 2 systems were calibrated to the ANSI-1989 audiometric standard (ANSI, 1989). Otoscopy and tympanometry . Otoscopy was performed to evaluate the status of the participants’ external ear canals and tympanic membranes. Middle ear function in six of seven participants was evaluated using a GSI-33 middle ear analyzer using a 226 Hz probe tone and a pump speed of -200 daPa/s. Data for the seventh participant (WS018) were obtained using a GSI 28 handheld tympanometer due to technical problems with the GSI-33 at the time of data collection. To avoid the possibility that participants’ concerns about this procedure would interfere with other aspects of data collection, these data were collected at the end of the assessment session. The “pass” for tympanometry for adults was a static admittance (Peak Y) occurring between +100 and -150 daPa and peak compliance 2 0.3 mmho, an equivalent ear-canal volume (Vec) between 0.6 and 1.5 ml (ASHA, 1990), and a gradient > 0.2 (Gelfand, 2001). The “pass” criteria for children was a static admittance occurring between +100 and -150 daPa and peak compliance 2 0.2 mmho, an equivalent volume (Vec) between 0.4 and 1.0 ml (ASHA, 1990), and a gradient > 0.2 (Nozza, Bluestone, Kardatzke, & Bachman, 1992). fieech audiometry The goal was to measure speech threshold and understanding of speech at suprathreshold levels in individuals with WS. Speech audiometry was performed with a GSI-16 audiometer using CD recordings prepared by Auditech of St. Louis. Standard 19 spondee lists were used to obtain speech recognition thresholds (SRT) for each ear, and the Northwestern University Auditory Test Number 6 (NU-6; Tillman & Carhart, 1966) lists were used to obtain word recognition scores for each ear. Stimuli for the word recognition tests were presented at 30 dB SL re: SRT for participant WS001; however, after the discovery of high frequency hearing loss in participant WSOOl, the presentation level was changed to 40 dB SL re: SRT, which is more efficient for both individuals with normal hearing and with hearing impairment (Gelfand, 2001). Both NU-6 and the Phonetically Balanced Kindergarten word lists (PBK-50; Haskins, 1949) were used to obtain word recognition scores for WS001. However, after working with children with WS at the national conference, it was hypothesized that given the verbal memory and language skill strengths of individuals with WS, even the children would be able to respond to the N U-6 list. The intent was to begin with an NU-6 list and shift to a PBK—50 list if necessary. As will be noted later, problems with factors such as fatigue precluded assessment of word recognition in the children who participated in the study. Otoacoustic emissions data The goal of this task was to investigate outer hair cell function in individuals with WS. Distortion product otoacoustic emissions (DPOAEs) were obtained using an Intelligent Hearing System (IHS) SmartOAE instrument. Data were collected for each ear in response to primary tones (labeled f1 and f2), with the ratio of these frequencies (f2/f 1) equal to 1.22. The frequency of f2 varied from 1,000 to 8,000 Hz in 1/8-octave steps. Twenty-five pairs of frequencies were presented and data from 16 sweeps were recorded and averaged for the response to each pair. Primary levels were set to f1 = 65 dB SPL and 20 f2 = 55 dB SPL. The criterion for an emission consistent with hearing loss was that it be below the 5‘h percentile for ears with normal hearing as reported by Gorga et al. (1997). Loudness discomfort level (LDL) rating The goal of this condition was to measure the loudness discomfort levels (LDLs) in individuals with WS and determine whether individuals with WS demonstrate lower LDLs for multi-talker babble and/or speech noise than would be expected in the general population. Speech noise generated by a GSI-16 audiometer was used for these measurements because for normally hearing individuals, speech-like signals yield lower thresholds of discomfort than do pure-tone signals (Dirks & Kamm, 1976). Thus, the impact of hyperacusis might be more readily observed with speech noise than with tones. In addition, the l2-talker multi-talker babble from the Revised Speech Perception in Noise Test (R-SPIN; Bilger, Nuetzel, & Rabinowitz, 1984) was used in this task to obtain the data to reflect real-world noisy communication situations. In order to investigate a broad range of tolerance levels, the softest stimulus was presented at 5 dB HL, and the loudest stimulus was presented at 65 dB HL. Due to the concern that individuals with WS would not be able to tolerate loud stimuli, an upper level of 65 dB HL (approximately 85 dB SPL) was selected, lower than the loudness discomfort level for speech stimuli (100 dB SPL) obtained from a normal—hearing population reported by Dirks & Kamm (1976). Stimuli were presented binaurally at ascending levels of 5 to 65 dB HL in 10-dB steps, using TDH-49 earphones. Participants were asked to rate the signals on a five-point scale. Due to WS subjects’ remarkable ability to process affect (Bellugi et al., 2000), and to recognize, discriminate, and remember unfamiliar and familiar faces (Rossen, Jones, Wang, & Klima, 1995), a picture . 21 with five iconic faces was provided for this task (Figure 1). This picture was revised from Kawell, Kopun, and Stelmachowicz ( 1988) by changing the facial expressions and the wording of the rating scale. The surprised face with the word “hurts” used by Kawell et al. (1988) was removed to prevent upsetting individuals with WS who report oversensitivity to loud sounds. The facial expression “just right” in Kawell et al. (1998) was moved from the second lowest level to a neutral place (Number 3) in the current study and the wording was changed to “OK.” The instructions for this task were Figure 1. Picture provided for LDL rating measure. “mm/om \/\/\\ <;:/\:/ CAN‘T T00 T00 SOFT 0. K. LOUD LOUD “You will hear different sounds. Point to the faces on this paper to show me how you feel. If you cannot hear it, just wait. If it is soft, point to this one (Number 2). If you feel comfortable, just like listening to music, point to this one (Number 3). If it is a little loud, but does not bother you, point to this one (Number 4). If it is too loud and you want me to turn it off, point to the last one (Number 5).” Signals were presented binaurally in a random order. Each signal level was presented twice. If the participant’s response on the second trial was not consistent with the first, then the level was presented a third time. The responses rated “5 (Too Loud)” were recorded as the individuals’ LDL to the stimulus. Participants’ responses to both speech noise and multi-talker babble were 22 obtained using this task. The data for the speech noise were collected first, and then the multi-talker babble data were collected. Hyperacusis questionnaire A hyperacusis questionnaire that was an expansion of the form used by Klein et al. (1994) was provided to the parents of each participant. This questionnaire was . constructed to obtain data on the prevalence of hyperacusis and otitis media, and was modified from Klein et al. by adding questions about the participant’s past and present responses to sounds in order to investigate the relation between age and hyperacusis. A copy of the form is available in Appendix A. This questionnaire was either completed during the data-collection session or mailed back afterward. In order to be able to compare the results with the Klein et al. data, questions regarding the participant’s behaviors covered hyperacusis and phonophobia together rather than as different responses to sounds. 23 CHAPTER 3 Results Audiological assessment Otoscopy For participants W800], WSOO2, W8003, WSOO4, WSOl8, and WS028, otoscopy revealed external canals within normal limits, no excessive cerumen and intact tympanic membranes. Otoscopy could not be performed on WSOl6 due to his unwillingness to allow the otoscope to be placed in his ears. Immitance Tympanograms were obtained for both ears in five of the seven participants, and the results from four of five showed normal static admittance, equivalent volume, gradient and compliance. Participant WSOOl ’3 had ear canal volumes greater than 90% of the ASHA (ASHA, 1990) data; however, his tympanograms suggested normal middle ear function. Tympanometry was accomplished only for the left ear in child participant WSOl6 and showed middle ear stiffness (flat tympanogram with ear canal volume 0.4 ml). Tympanometry for the right ear in W8016 could not be conducted due to the lack of a good seal and his unwillingness to continue this task. Tympanometry was attempted with child participant W8028, but could not be conducted because of her unwillingness to allow the probe to be placed in her ears. The individual data of Immitance measures are listed in the Appendix B. Pure-tone audiometry Air-conduction and bone-conduction thresholds for stimuli from 500 to 8,000 Hz for each participant are shown in Figure 2. Standard pure-tone thresholds were obtained 24 from all the participants, and extended high-frequency pure-tone thresholds were obtained from adult participant WSOO4. Thresholds Z 20 dB HL at any frequency in adults and 15 dB HL at any frequency in children were considered to indicate hearing loss. The criterion for identifying hearing loss as sensorineural was that there be no more than a single air-bone-gap Z 10 dB HL in the pure-tone thresholds. The degrees of hearing impairment were defined using the categories recommended by Jerger and Jerger (1980) for adult participants and by Northern and Downs (2002) for child participants. Five of the seven participants (WSOOI, WSOOZ, WSOO4, WSOl 8, and WSO28) in the study demonstrated bilateral sensorineural hearing loss and three of those five participants (WSOOI , WSOO2, and WSOlS) showed a pattern of high frequency hearing loss. Figure 2. Hearing thresholds for each participant (0: Right ear air-conduction thresholds; X: Left ear air-conduction thresholds; <: Unmasked right ear bone-conduction thresholds; >: Unmasked left bone-conduction thresholds; [: Masked right ear bone-conduction thresholds; ]: Masked left ear bone-conduction thresholds). Frequency in Hz 250 500 1000 2000 4000 8000 -1D 10 4%\;\ k ‘r Hearing thresholds in dB HL (ANSI 1989) 100 % 11o ‘ i ‘ ‘ WS001 25 Figure 2 (continued). Hearing thresholds for each participant (0: Right ear air-conduction thresholds; X: Left ear air-conduction thresholds; <: Unmasked right ear bone-conduction thresholds; >: Unmasked left bone-conduction thresholds; [: Masked right ear bone- conduction thresholds; ]: Masked left ear bone-conduction thresholds). Frequency in Hz 250 500 1000 2000 4000 8000 '10 . E 3 z s 0 f: ,? L a) ‘ = i 5 r: 10 {pm RX? 13 (3 20 5 = Q>)<‘X\ 5" 5\o "I" 30 : 33k % 4° * 2\ c é 5° ‘ x 25 60 - ‘ .5 g 70 5 o, 80 .s a 90 03 I100 - e - 110 WS002 Frequency in Hz 10 250 500 1000 2000 4000 8000 ° 37%m\<>7&_ / . 10 <fl§ VI -\X/g Hearing thresholds in dB HL (ANSl 1989) 110 WSOO3 26 Figure 2 (continued). Hearing thresholds for each participant (0: Right ear air-conduction thresholds; X: Left ear air-conduction thresholds; <: Unmasked right ear bone-conduction thresholds; >: Unmasked left bone-conduction thresholds; [: Masked right ear bone- conduction thresholds; ]: Masked left ear bone-conduction thresholds). Frequency in Hz 250 500 1000 2000 4000 8000 -10 0 10 20 Hearing thresholds in dB HL (ANSI 1989) 5‘ 8 8 s 8888 1 WSOO4 Frequency in Hz 250 500 1000 2000 4000 8000 Hearing thresholds in dB HL (ANSI 1989) WS016 27 Figure 2 (continued). Hearing thresholds for each participant (0: Right ear air-conduction thresholds; X: Left ear air-conduction thresholds; <: Unmasked right ear bone-conduction thresholds; >: Unmasked left bone-conduction thresholds; [: Masked right ear bone- conduction thresholds; ]: Masked left ear bone-conduction thresholds). Frequency inHz 10 250 500 1000 2000 4000 8000 a o :< = :10 i< IL,“ g 20 g/n . X\\;< ,0 3 s : *\ a “8 4° X r: 5 i 5 i 3”; 5° ’ ' i '5 60 -= :6 70 : 5 or 80 T .E ' g 90 I100 110 , VVS018 Frequency inHz 10 250 500 1000 2000 4000 8000 a? ‘ 5 5 co 0 e I ‘ a) a g 10 > 1 :/)u 3.; 20 <>\ ,,a;x— \ s 33‘ Xfix: 98 4° : C'. : E: 5" '5 60 f 4: s 3 70 ; 5 on 80 l .E a 90 0 I100 110 WS028 28 Participant WS001 demonstrated hearing within normal limits through 2,000 Hz in both ears. Thresholds above that point were indicative of a mild, sloping sensorineural hearing loss in the right ear and a mild-to-moderate, sloping, sensorineural hearing loss in the left ear. False-positive responses were only observed when high-frequency stimuli were presented and re-instruction was provided when they occurred. Participant WSOOl was one of the two individuals in the current study who reported presence of tinnitus during the test. It is possible that participant W800] had difficulty discriminating between the pure-tone stimuli and his tinnitus. The reliability of his pure-tone data was judged to be good. Pure-tone thresholds for participant WS002’s right ear were within normal limits through 4,000 Hz, and then indicated mild sensorineural hearing loss at 8,000 Hz. Left ear pure-tone thresholds were within normal limits through 2,000 Hz, and were indicative of mild-to-moderate, sloping, sensorineural hearing loss. The reliability of her pure-tone data was judged to be good. Participant W8002 showed a bone-conduction threshold poorer than air-conduction thresholds (i.e., a reverse air-bone-gap) at 4,000 Hz in both ears. The air-conduction and bone-conduction thresholds are within ilO dB of each other; therefore, “it is possible and perfectly acceptable for a bone-conduction threshold to be a bit poorer than air-conduction thresholds at the same frequency, even if everything is done correctly” (Gelfand, 2001, p. 155-156). However, the 20 dB reverse air-bone-gap at 4,000 Hz in her right ear was not typical. The placement of the bone oscillator was checked and the thresholds at 4,000 Hz were re-tested during the data collection in order to rule out technical errors. It is important to note that participant W8002 also reported the presence of ringing in her ears during the test. It is possible that 29 participant WS002 had difficulty discriminating between the pure—tone stimuli and her tinnitus. Participant WSOO3 was the only participant in the current study who showed normal pure-tone thresholds across the entire test frequency range in both ears. The reliability of his pure-tone data was judged to be good. Although the audiogram of WSOO3 showed a 15 dB air-bone gap at 4,000 Hz in the left ear, tympanograms for both of W8003’s ears revealed normal middle ear function. Therefore, the air-bone-gap at 4,000 Hz in the left ear was not considered to indicate the presence of a conductive component to the hearing loss. Participant WSOO4 demonstrated a bilateral mild (500 to 2,000 Hz) to moderate (above 2,000 Hz) sensorineural hearing loss. He showed some inconsistent responses to the stimuli during this task and re-instruction was provided when false—positive responses occurred. The reliability of his pure-tone data was judged to be only fair due to his inconsistent responses. One child (WS016) showed a left unilateral hearing loss of an unidentified type. Tympanometric data for that ear were indicative of middle ear pathology, and mixed hearing loss could not be ruled out without bone-conduction data. Bone-conduction data were not collected because the child lost interest in the task and could not be coaxed to continue. The reliability of his pure-tone data was judged to be fair. Pure-tone thresholds for participant WS018’s left ear showed mild, rising, sensorineural hearing loss at 500 to 2,000 Hz, and then indicated mild-to-moderate, sloping, sensorineural hearing loss range in the high frequencies. Air-conduction thresholds for the right ear were obtained only from 1,000 to 8,000 Hz due to the 30 participant’s loss of interest in the task. Right ear pure-tone thresholds showed a mild sensorineural hearing loss at 1,000 Hz, then a rise to the normal hearing range at 2,000 Hz, and finally a slope to mild hearing loss range at 4,000 to 8,000 Hz. The reliability of his pure-tone responses was judged to be good. No bone-conduction threshold was obtained at 500 Hz due to participant WSOl8’s loss of interest in the task. Sensorineural hearing loss at 500 Hz in the left ear was confirmed by the normal immitance results for WSOl8. Pure-tone thresholds for participant WS028’s right ear showed mild-to-moderate, sloping, sensorineural hearing loss from 500 to 1,000 Hz, then a rise to mild, essentially flat, sensorineural hearing loss at 2,000 to 6,000 Hz, and finally a rise to normal—hearing range at 8,000 Hz. Left ear pure-tone thresholds showed mild to moderate, essentially flat, sensorineural hearing loss at 500 to 1,000 Hz, then a rise to mild, essentially flat, sensorineural hearing loss at 2,000 to 8,000 Hz. Due to WSO28’s fear of earphone placement and refusal to stay in the testing booth at the beginning of the audiological evaluation, the procedure was modified to help participant WSO28 gradually overcome her fear. Play audiometry, using a Beltone 120 portable audiometer, was performed during the language evaluation session to help the participant get used to the earphone placement. WS028 was conditioned to put a block in a puzzle board when she heard pulsed tones through the earphones. VRA using FM signal (warble tones) was introduced after play audiometry to help her become comfortable with the testing booth. After this, participant WS028 was able to do play audiometry (using pulsed tones) in the testing booth, and her pure-tone data were collected using the G51 61 audiometer. The reliability of her pure-tone data was judged to be good. 31 Extended high-frequency (EHF) data were obtained only for WSOO4, and they were converted to dB SPL by adding the reference equivalent threshold sound pressure levels (RETSPLs) recommended by Frank (2001). The test reliability was judged to be fair due to his false positive responses to stimuli. Table 3 shows the data comparison of EHF air-conduction thresholds of WSOO4 and the mean data of 32 individuals (16 male and 16 female) aged 20 to 30 years reported by Stelmachowicz, Beauchaine, Kalberer and Jesteadt (1989). These data were used for comparison because they were obtained from a population that included the age range of the adult participants in the current study (i.e., 240 individuals in the range of 10 to 60 years), the subgroups of each age range contained equal numbers of male and female individuals, and all of the individuals were reported to have normal middle ear function and a negative history of excessive noise exposure or ototoxic drug use. The data comparison shows that W8004’s EHF thresholds were 56.6 dB or greater than the mean data for individuals aged 20 to 30 years reported by Stelmachowicz et al. (1989). Table 3. Data comparison of extended high-frequency air-conduction thresholds obtained from WSOO4 and mean thresholds for 32 individuals age 20 to 30 years reported by Stelmachowicz et a1. (1989). Right Ear (dB SPL) Left ear (dB SPL) Frequency (kHz) 10 12.5 16 10 12.5 16 WSOO4 87.6 NR NR 112.6 NR NR Stelmachowicz et al., 1989 31 38 72 31 38 72 NR: No response when stimuli reached the maximum audiometer output. Speech audiometg Speech recognition thresholds (SRT) and word recognition scores (WRS) were obtained from the four adult participants, ages 17 to 43 years. Pure-tone averages were calculated using the thresholds for 500, 1,000, and 2,000 Hz, the SRT data, and the WRS 32 data are shown in Table 4. Each of the adult participants demonstrated excellent word recognition as indicated by scores 2 92%. Speech audiometry could not be conducted with three child participants due to their loss of attention to the task. Table 4. Speech audiometry and pure-tone-average data for the four adults who completed the speech audiometry tasks. Right Ear Left Ear PTA SRT WRS PTA SRT WRS Presentation % Presentation % level (dB HL) Correct level (dB HL) Correct WSA001 12 dB 15 dB 45 dB 98 12 dB 15 dB 45 dB 98 WSA002 8 dB 5 dB 45 dB 100 7 dB 0 dB . 40 dB 100 WSA003 5 dB 10 dB 50 dB 96 3 dB 5 dB 45 dB 96 WSA004 38 dB 25 dB 65 dB 100 33 dB 25 dB 65 dB 92 Data for three of the four participants indicate consistency between pure-tone averages (PTA) and SRTs (i.e., a difference that is S 6-12 dB; Brandy, 2002). The fourth participant’s data were consistent for the left ear but differed by 13 dB in the right ear. Participant WSOO4 demonstrated some inconsistent responses to pure-tone stimuli and needed more re-instructions than the other participants during the pure-tone audiometric measurement. It is possible that this contributed to a slightly elevated pure-tone average. Any inconsistent responses (differing by more than 5 dB) can lead to a different result in terms of the consistency between PTA and SRT. This individual also had difficulty convincing his family members that he has hearing loss. Therefore, it is possible that WSOO4 exaggerated the severity of hearing loss in order to convince his family that he is hearing impaired. 33 Otoacoustic emissions data DPOAE data were obtained from five of the seven individuals (WS001, WSOOZ, WSOO3, WS004, and W8018). DPOAE measures were attempted with the two remaining child participants (WSOl6 and WS028), but they could not be conducted because of the children’s unwillingness to allow the probe to be placed in their ears. DPOAE data for the participants in this study are shown in Figure 3. The 95'h percentile data for DPOAE amplitude for hearing-impaired ears and the 5‘h percentile data for DPOAE amplitude for normally hearing ears, as reported by Gorga et al. (1997) are plotted in Figure 3 for comparison. All points below the Gorga et al. data (i.e., at a lower dB SPL) have a significant probability of coming from cars with hearing loss. Data above the 95th percentile for hearing-impaired ears are considered indicative of hearing within normal limits. Data between the 5‘h percentile for normally hearing ears and the 95lh percentile for hearing-impaired ears are considered difficult to interpret, and therefore ambiguous. Of the five data sets shown in Figure 3, one was from an individual who had hearing within normal limits (WSOO3) and four were from individuals who demonstrated bilateral sensorineural hearing loss. The DPOAE data support the audiological finding of hearing loss in three (WSOOl, WSOO2, WSOl8.) of the four participants who showed bilateral hearing loss. Participant WSOOl demonstrated pure-tone thresholds that sloped from within normal limits to the mild (right) or moderate (left) hearing loss range from 4,000 to 8,000 Hz. His DPOAE amplitudes in the frequency range of 1,500 to 3,000 Hz were indicative of normal hearing. DPOAE amplitudes fell below the 5‘h percentile of the Gorga et al. 34 data in the frequency range of 4,000 to 6,000 Hz, and thus were consistent with hearing- impaired ears. Thirty-eight percent of the DPOAE responses from the right ear and 33% of the responses from the left ear fell within the ambiguous range. Participant WS002 also demonstrated thresholds that sloped from within normal limits to the mild (right) or moderate (left) hearing loss range at 8,000 and/or 4,000 to 6,000 Hz. Most of the DPOAE amplitudes above 2,000 Hz in her right car were indicative of normal hearing. Her DPOAE amplitudes fell below the 5‘h percentile of Gorga et al. data in the frequency range of 6,000 to 7,000 Hz in the left ear, and thus were consistent with hearing—impaired ears. Thirty-three percent of the DPOAE responses from the right ear and 42% of the responses from the left ear fell within the ambiguous range. Figure 3. Individual DPOAES for participants WS001, W5002, WSOO3, WS004, and WS018. Mean DPOAE Noise —xi WSO1 LE —0— W801 RE —I— Gorga et al 5th Percentile -v— Gorga et al 95th Percentile 20— DPOAE Amplitude (dB SPL) l I l l T l l 1000 2000 3000 4000 5000 6000 70008000 Frequency (Hz) 35 Figure 3 (continued). Individual DPOAES for participants WS001, W8002, WSOO3, WS004, and WS018. Mean DPOAE Noise —><— W803 LE —0 — W803 RE —I— Gorga et at 5th Percentile —v— Gorga et at 95th Percentile 20- DPOAE Amplitude (dB SPL) ' r l " ' l l 9 l l l l 1000 2000 3000 4000 5000 6000 70008000 Frequency (Hz) Mean DPOAE Noise . —x— W802 LE —0— W802 RE —-— Gorga et al 5th Percentile —v— Gorga et al 95th Percentile DPOAE Amplitude (dB SPL) A l l I I l I l 1000 2000 3000 4000 5000 6000 70005000 Frequency (Hz) 36 Figure 3 (continued). Individual DPOAES for participants WS001, WS002, WS003, WS004, and WS018. — Mean DPOAE Noise —x— W804 LE 20 l —0— W804 RE ‘1 —I— Gorga et at 5th Percentile A —v~ Gorga et al 95th Percentile ..l n. _ w 10 in E 8 o - .3 'a E < -10 _ u < O a D -20 - -30 .. ' I l i l l l l l 1000 2000 3000 4000 5000 6000 70008000 Frequency (Hz) —* Mean DPOAE Noise —x— W818 LE 20 —0— W818 RE q ——I— Gorga et al 5th Percentile A '1 —v— Gorga et al 95th Percentile .1 0. _ <0 10 m B 8 o - 3 ’a E < -10 _ Lu < O n. o -20 — -30 I I l l l l l 1000 2000 3000 4000 5000 6000 70008000 Frequency (Hz) 37 Participant WS003’s pure-tone thresholds were within normal limits bilaterally. His DPOAE amplitudes in the frequency range of 1,500 to 3.000 H2 and above 5,000 Hz were indicative of normal hearing in both ears. Forty-six percent of the DPOAE responses from the right ear and 42% of the responses from the left ear fell within the ambiguous range. Participant WSOO4’s pure-tone thresholds indicated bilateral mild-to-moderate hearing loss across the stimulus frequency range. His DPOAE amplitudes between 1,500 and 3,000 Hz were indicative of normal hearing and DPOAE amplitudes above 5,000 Hz were consistent with hearing-impaired ears. Forty—six percent of the DPOAE responses from the right ear and 50% of the responses from the left ear fell within the ambiguous range. Participant W8018 demonstrated thresholds that rose from mild hearing loss (at 1,000 Hz) to normal-hearing range (at 2,000 Hz), and then sloped to mild hearing loss in the right ear. She also demonstrated thresholds that sloped from mild-to-moderate hearing loss (above 4,000 Hz) in the left ear. The DPOAE amplitudes in the frequency range of 2,000 to 2,500 Hz and at frequencies above 4,500 Hz in her right car were indicative of normal hearing. The DPOAE amplitudes in the frequency range of 5,500 to 6,500 Hz in her left car were consistent with hearing impaired ears. Fifty-four percent of the DPOAE responses from the right ear and 75% of the responses from the left ear fell within the ambiguous range. Test-retest reliability Test-retest reliability was assessed by comparing two sets of pure-tone threshold data and DPOAE data for the youngest participant in the current study (WS018). Data 38 from the current study were compared to data obtained at the 2004 Williams Syndrome Association Conference two months earlier. It is important to note that the subjects for the study of Marler et a1. (2004) and the subjects for this project do not overlap (i.e., because the subject had follow-up assessment for this project, she was not included in the conference screening data set reported). Table 5 provides the pure-tone data comparison. Due to environmental constraints (i.e., ambient noise levels), the criterion for passing a hearing screening at the conference was 20 dB HL. A threshold search was conducted only for those frequencies at which the individual failed the screening. Differences between thresholds obtained at the conference and thresholds obtained at the clinic were within 5 dB HL. This is considered to be typical test-retest variation (Harrell, 2002). Table 5. Participant WS018’s air-conduction thresholds obtained from hearing screening and diagnostic evaluation. Right ear (dB HL) Left ear (dB HL) Freq. (Hz) 500 1k 2k 4k 6k 8k 500 1k 2k 4k 6k 8k 5:;ference Pass Pass Pass 30 25 Pass 25 Pass Pass 30 45 40 2:30” 20 15 10 25 25 15 25 15 15 25 40 40 “Pass” equals to a response to 20 dB HL stimuli. Figure 4 provides the DPOAE data comparison. The DPOAE parameter settings in Marler et al. were the same as the current study. These two sets of data show a similar pattern and are consistent with audiometric findings. Sladen, Lamb and Hall (1996, as cited in Hall, J. W. III, 2000) reported that the intrasubject DPOAE amplitude variability was typically between 6 and 7 dB and as low as 3 dB SPL. The intrasubject amplitude variance for DPOAE by F2 frequency was within the dB range reported by Sladen et al. (1996) for 16 of 24 data points, including all of the points in the region between 4,000 and 6,000 Hz, the region in which data are consistent with hearing loss. 39 Figure 4. DPOAES of participant W8018 obtained from hearing screening and diagnostic evaluation. --—- DPOAE Noise Floor 25 _ —I —— Gorga 5th Percentile for Hearing Impaired Ears -- X-- W818 LE Conference DPOAE 2° ‘ —o—ws1e RE Conference DPOAE 15 1 '* ‘V- Gorga 95m Percentile for Normal Hearing Ears o-o—°~o\ 00, Absolute DPOAE Intensity (dB SPL) I ' Tfi‘ .fi—fi' ' ‘7 1— 1000 2000 3000 4000 5000 6000 70008000 F2 Frequency (Hz) : *7 DPOAE Noise Floor 25 _ * l — Gorga 5th Percentile for Hearing Impaired Ears -- X-- W818 LE Clinic DPOAE 2°] —0— wsw RE Clinic DPOAE 15 .1 —V— Gorga 95th Percentile for Normal Hearing Ears Absolute DPOAE Intensity (dB SPL) .30 ___ v... __ _ _. *_: —I' l_‘“ T' "'1” ‘- 1000 2000 3000 4000 5000 6000 70008000 FZFrequncy (Hz) 40 Loudness discomfort level (LDL) rating LDL rating data were obtained from four adult participants (WS001, WSOOZ, WSOO3, and WSOO4). The rating system was changed from a three-item scale to a five- item scale after the data of Participant W800] were collected. Therefore, the LDL data from WSOOl were excluded from the data analysis. The LDL data were not collected from any of the children due to the participants’ fatigue and loss of attention to the task. The data for WS002, WSOO3 and WSOO4 are shown in Table 6. Table 6. Loudness discomfort level rating scale data for the adult participants Speech noise stimulus level (dB HL) Multi-talker babble stimulus level (dB HL) 15 25 35 45 55 65 5 15 25 35 45 55 65 5 WSOOZ 2 2 3 3 3 5 l 2 3 3 5 5 W8003 l 3 4 3 5 4 5 2 2 5 5 WSO04 l 2 3 3 3 4 4 l 2 3 4 1=cannot hear; 2=too soft; 3=O.K.; 4=loud, but tolerable; 5:100 loud. Participant WS002, who showed a mild sensorineural hearing loss at 8,000 Hz in the right ear, and a mild-to-moderate sensorineural hearing loss above the 4,000 Hz range in the left ear, rated the speech noise stimuli as “too loud” at 55 dB HL and the multi- talker babble as “too loud” at 45 and 55 dB HL. Participant WSOO3, who showed hearing within normal limits in pure-tone audiometry, rated the multi-talkcr babble as “too loud” at 45 dB HL, but was inconsistent in identifying the discomfort level for the speech-noise stimuli (i.e., rated 45 dB HL as “too loud,” but rated 55 dB HL as “loud, but tolerable”). Participant WSOO4, who showed a bilateral mild-to-moderate sensorineural hearing loss, rated both the speech noise and the multi-talker babble stimuli as “loud, but tolerable” at 65 dB HL. Due to the predetermined upper limit of 65 dB HL in this task, the investigator could not determine the level that participant WSOO4 would have rated “too loud.” 41 .3. ‘-..---.§.A.' "‘5 Of the three individuals who performed the LDL tasks, two (WSOO2 and WSOO4) responded consistently to the stimuli and two (WSOO2 and WSOO3) demonstrated LDLs lower than the predetermined upper limit. Although limited, these findings were consistent with expectations prior to the initiation of the study. The following unexpected findings were also observed. Participant WSOOZ showed bilateral high-frequency sensorineural hearing loss, and her LDL was lower than that of WSOO3, who showed pure-tone thresholds within normal limits. Participant WSOO3 demonstrated inconsistent ratings of the stimuli. Participant WS004’s actual LDL was presumably higher than the upper limit allowed by the research design. Hyperacusis questionnaire Hypersensitivity to sounds During or after data collection, all of the parents of the participants returned questionnaires regarding the individual’s responses to loud sounds and history of hearing loss, and middle ear disease. All of them reported that their children were “bothered or frightened” by certain sounds during childhood and that all of them, including those now adults, were still “bothered or frightened” by these sounds when this survey was conducted. The following items were those most commonly selected by the parents as the sounds that cause the greatest discomfort: “firecracker (six out of seven),” “fire engine siren (five out of seven),” and “power saw (four out of seven)” Additional sounds selected by some parents as the sounds that caused the greatest discomfort to their 99 66 children were “motorcycle,” “loud auto muffler,” “electric drill,” “dog barking, lawn mower,” “squeaking toys,” “hammering a nail,” and “food blender.” 42 The parents of three participants (WS001, WSOO3, and WSOl6) reported that the discomfort level associated with loud sounds became reduced as their children grew up; the parents of the remaining participants reported that the discomfort level remained the same. Only the father of individual W8003 reported that he had looked for professional help for his child’s hyperacusis. That parent noted that his child liked to play music and did not complain about its intensity. The parent of participant WSOO4 reported that his child likes car races, so he wears ear protection and forces himself to tolerate the loud noises. When responding to a question focused only on “fear,” the parents of participant WS001 and participant WSOO4 reported that these individuals continue to dislike the types of sounds that were reported earlier (e. g., firecracker, fire engine siren, power saw), but that these sounds no longer cause fear. The parents of the youngest participant (WSOl8) reported that their daughter still had fear of these sounds; however, during the experimental session, they stated that her hyperacusis had never been as disruptive as reported by other parents of children with WS. The parents of the remaining adult and child participants reported that fear of loud sounds was still an issue at the time this survey was conducted. The following descriptions of the participants’ most frequent responses to loud sounds were noted in the parent reports. Six out of seven sets of parents reported that the participant “covers ears with hands.” Five out of seven reported “cries,” five out of seven reported “cringes” and four out of seven reported “says something like I don’t like it.” All of the parents reported that the characteristics of sounds that most bothered the participants were how loud the sound is and a sudden sound. 43 Hearing Six of seven sets of parents reported that no diagnosis of permanent hearing loss had been made before this study. The seventh, the parent of W8018 did not answer this question. During the evaluation session, the parent of W8018 reported that her child failed a hearing screening in kindergarten and that no follow-up diagnostic testing had been done prior to the current study. She was unable to provide any additional details regarding the hearing loss. The parents of W8018 also reported that hearing loss was not a noticeable problem in their daughter. Participant WSOO4 reported a history of hearing loss and rarely had middle ear infections; however, his parent reported that he had no diagnosis of permanent hearing loss and that he was “obsessive about being deaf.” His parent also reported (as noted above) that participant WSOO4 likes to attend car races and forces himself to tolerate loud noise by using earplugs. Participant WSOO4 also reported that he understands the importance of using hearing protection when exposed to noise and that he uses earplugs to protect his hearing. Middle ear infection Two of seven sets of parents reported that their children (W8002 and WSOO3) never had ear infections, and three additional sets reported that their children (W8001, WSOO4, and W8018) rarely had ear infections. W8004’s parents reported that he never had an ear infection in childhood, and rarely has one now..The remaining two sets of parents reported that their children (W8016 and W8028) had recurrent ear infections (several or constant) during childhood, but that they were less frequent as the participants grew up. 44 CHAPTER 4 Discussion Audiolggical assessment At the initiation of the current project, the prevalence of hearing loss in individuals with W8 had not been widely studied. Due to the characteristic of hypersensitivity to sounds, most individuals with W8 have been assumed to have normal or better than normal hearing. In fact, data reported by Ruangdaraganon et al. (1999) and Johnson et al. (2001) have shown that individuals with W8 have a higher prevalence of hearing loss (33%) than is demonstrated by the children ages 3 to 10 years (9 in 10,000) (Van Naarden, Decoufle, and Caldwell, 1999). In addition, hearing screening and DPOAE data obtained by Marler et al. (2004) during a Williams Syndrome Association conference revealed that a surprising number of individuals with W8 (74% of those examined) had poorer-than-expected auditory function. A report published by Cherniske et al. (2004) during the course of the current study confirms and expands previous reports of hearing loss in adults with W8. Cherniske et al. investigated multisystem function (i.e., cognitive, cardiovascular, musculoskeletal, gastrointestinal, endocrine, visual and auditory systems functioning) in 20 adults with W8 over 30 years of age. Sixteen of these 20 adults had “standard audiological testing” (p. 256) in a hospital setting. Seventy-five percent had high-frequency sensorineural hearing loss. Half of these hearing losses were unilateral. In the current study, five of seven participants had bilateral symmetrical sensorineural hearing loss. The configurations were either hearing loss only in the high 45 frequency range or hearing loss across the entire stimulus range, with thresholds greater (poorer) above 2,000 Hz than below 2,000 Hz. Figure 5 compares the four adult participants’ air-conduction thresholds to the median threshold data for the general population calculated using data and formulas provided by ISO 7029-1984. The ISO 7029-1984 data provides the audiometric information for the frequency range of 125 to 8,000 Hz from the otologically normal 1? individuals aged range of 18 to 70 years. According to ISO 7029-1984, the criteria for identifying otologically normal was “a person in a normal state of health who at the time ii of testing is free from excess wax in the ear canals, is without known ear pathology and r who has no history of undue exposure of noise” (p. 2). The report also provides formulas for calculating the expected value of the median hearing threshold shift relative to a group of persons aged 18 years. The median thresholds in Figure 5 were generated from data sets selected according to each participant’s age and gender. All of the air-conduction thresholds for individuals W800] and WSOO4 were equal to or poorer than the median 180 7029-1984 thresholds. The pure-tone thresholds of W8002 were equal to or poorer than the median thresholds with three exceptions. The left ear threshold for 500 Hz was 1.6 dB better than the median and the thresholds for both ears at 1,000 Hz were 1.83 dB better than the median. The data from W8003 were equal or poorer than median thresholds with just two exceptions. Thresholds from both ears at 2,000 Hz were 5 dB better than the median. This data comparison shows that four of the adult participants had at least three thresholds in each ear that were 15 to 55 dB poorer than would be expected, given age and gender. For W800] and W8002, these differences were greatest above 2,000 Hz. 46 Figure 5. Comparison of the four adult participants’ air-conduction thresholds to expected median thresholds. The median was calculated from data and formulas provided by ISO 7029-1984 according to each individual’s age and gender (0: Right ear air-conduction thresholds; X: Left ear air-conduction thresholds; *: Median threshold data calculated from ISO 7029—1984). Frequency in Hz 250 500 1000 2000 4000 8000 -10 0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 Hearing thresholds in dB HL (ANSI 1989) W800] (37 y/o, male) Frequency in Hz 10 250 500 1000 2000 4000 8000 Hearing thresholds in dB HL (ANSI 1989) W8002 (29 y/o, female) 47 Figure 5 (continued). Comparison of the four adult participants’ air-conduction thresholds to expected median thresholds. The median was calculated from data and formulas provided by ISO 7029-1984 according to each individual’s age and gender (0: Right ear air-conduction thresholds; X: Left ear air-conduction thresholds; *: Median threshold data calculated from ISO 71129-1984). Frequency in Hz 250 500 1000 2000 4000 8000 -10 10 20 . 8 70 80 Hearing thresholds in dB HL (ANSI 1989) 100 110 WSOO3 (17.5 y/o, male) Frequency in Hz 250 500 1000 2000 4000 8000 0 L gaL la Hearing thresholds in dB HL (ANSI 1989) 8 r \. A 9 mm (26 y/o, male) 48 Thresholds for WSOO4 were 30 to 55 dB poorer than would be expected given age and gender at all the test frequencies. Figure 6 compares the three child participants’ air-conduction thresholds to the median threshold data that was reported by Holmes, Niskar, Kieszak, Rubin, and Brody (1994). Holmes et al. obtained hearing thresholds in the frequency range of 500 to 8,000 Hz from 6,166 children ages 6 to 19 years in the Third National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey. In the study of Holmes et al., physical examinations and home interviews were performed to determine test exclusions (e.g., draining ears, colds, sinus problems, or exposure to loud noise over the past 24 hours). This study provides the mean and median thresholds by ear, gender, frequency and age group (6 to 11 years, 12 to 19 year). The median thresholds from the age group that matched each child participant’s age were used for data comparison in Figure 6. All of the air-conduction thresholds for individual W8016 and W8018 were poorer than'the median thresholds reported by Holmes et al. The pure-tone threshold at 8,000 Hz in the right ear of participant WSO28 was 8 dB better than the median thresholds for the female ages 7 to 11 years in Holmes et al. Only one pure-tone threshold at one frequency in one participant was observed to be better than those of normal hearing age-peers. With the exception of isolated thresholds from three individuals (W8002, WSOO3, and W8028), all the participants in the current study showed pure-tone thresholds poorer than would be expected from age peers in the general population. Five of the seven participants in the current study showed bilateral sensorineural hearing loss. These 49 findings indicate that the prevalence of hearing loss in individuals with W8 is greater than previously reported. Although six of seven participants in the current study had no prior documented diagnosis of permanent hearing loss, four of these six participants showed sensorineural hearing loss. In addition, parental reports in the current study showed that most (six out of seven sets) of the participants’ parents were not aware of the possibility of hearing loss in individuals with WS. They reported assumptions that their family members with W8 would not have hearing impairment because of their oversensitive responses to sounds. These findings provide clear evidence that hearing impairment in individuals with W8 has often been overlooked. Figure 6. Comparison of the three child participants’ air-conduction thresholds to the median thresholds reported by Holmes et al. (1994). The median was selected according to each participant’s age and gender (0: Right ear air-conduction thresholds; X: Left ear air- conduction thresholds; *: Median threshold data reported by Holmes et al.). Frequency in Hz 250 500 1000 2000 4000 8000 -10 5 L .2 ; ,L/XQ 0 :\x J Y 10 f 20 Hearing thresholds in dB HL (ANSI 1989) 110 W8016 (7 y/o, male) 50 Figure 6 (continued). Comparison of the three child participants’ air-conduction thresholds to the median thresholds reported by Holmes et al. (1994). The median was selected according to each participant’s age and gender (0: Right ear air-conduction thresholds; X: Left ear air-conduction thresholds; 10 888 60 70 Hearing thresholds in dB HL (ANSI 1989) 100 110 Hearing thresholds in dB HL (ANSI 1989) 250 500 0 *: Median threshold data reported by Holmes et al.). Frequency in Hz 1000 2000 4000 8000 4"?” E/CP X/ Jam I\- X 250 500 W8018 (7.5 y/o, female) Frequency in Hz 1000 2000 4000 8000 . I I I _,_.__.'.._...-— 9 : I l A W8028 (11.5 y/o, female) 5] Previous studies have shown the impact of hearing impairment on children's language development, academic achievements and social behaviors. Davis, Elfenbein, Schum and Bentler (1986) investigated the effects of degree of hearing impairment, age, and other factors on intellectual, social, academic, and language behaviors on 40 children with mild to moderately severe hearing impairment. According to Davis et a1. (1986) “children with any degree of hearing loss appear to be at risk for delayed language development of verbal skills and reduced academic achievement” (p. 61). The researchers also reported that the two children with the mildest hearing loss in their study demonstrated some of the greatest vocabulary delays (i.e., 3 to 4 years on the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test - Revised). Bess, Dodd-Murphy and Parker (1998) defined minimal sensorineural hearing loss for three groups: unilateral sensorineural hearing loss (USHL, pure-tone average of 20 dB HL or greater in the impaired ear), bilateral sensorineural hearing loss (BSHL, pure-tone average between 20 dB and 40 dB HL bilaterally), and high-frequency sensorineural hearing loss (HFSHL, air—conduction thresholds greater than 25 dB HL at two or more frequencies above 2,000 Hz in one or both ears). They reported that 5.4 % of the children in the schools they worked with exhibited minimal sensorineural hearing loss (MSHL) and that 37% of children with MSHL failed at least one grade compared to a district rate of about 3%. Two of the three child participants (W8018 and W8028) in the current study met the Bess et a]. definition of MSHL. Participant W8018 exhibited HFSHL in her left ear and participant W8028 demonstrated BSHL. Not only did these two child participants exhibit MSHL, but also two of the four adult participants (W8001 and W8002) demonstrated HFSHL. It is clear that because of MSHL, these two child 52 Fri—fl participants could be experiencing greater difficulties in school than do their normal- hearing age-peers. Both the Davis et al. and Bess et al. studies revealed the importance of identification of hearing loss in general populations of children; children with W8 would be no different. It is important to note that minimal sensorineural hearing loss could not be revealed by standard school hearing screenings recommended by the ASHA Audiology Panel (1997) which uses a 20 dB HL presentation level for pure-tone stimuli at 1,000, 4J1 In m—fiy 2,000 and 4,000 Hz. Therefore, without a full hearing evaluation in a clinical setting, the possibility of ignorance of hearing loss is high. L The current study showed a high prevalence of hearing loss in the individuals with W8 and low awareness of this hearing loss among their parents. Two of the three child participants in the current study demonstrated minimal sensorineural hearing loss. There is also evidence that minimal sensorineural hearing loss could have been missed by standard school hearing screenings. Thus, an annual hearing evaluation should be included in the routine test of the individuals with WS. This is especially important because we do not yet have evidence of age-of-onset patterns for this population. Otoacoustic emission data The DPOAE measures of two (W8001 and WSOO4) of the five participants with bilateral hearing loss showed significantly reduced DPOAE amplitudes relative to data provided by Gorga et a1. (1997) for the high frequency range (i.e., from 4,000 to 6,000 Hz). Reduced DPOAE amplitudes, consistent with impaired outer hair cell function, were observed from 4,000 to 7,000 Hz in participant W800] and above 5,000 Hz in participant WSOO4. Marler et al. (2004) recently reported that 74% of their participants showed 53 reduced DPOAE amplitude in the frequency range of 3,500-6,000 Hz. Impaired outer hair cell function results in sensorineural hearing loss. Therefore, the findings of both the current study and the Marler et al. study are consistent with the findings of sensorineural hearing loss (75%) reported by Cherniske et a1. (2004). Only one participant in the current study demonstrated hearing within normal limits. He did not demonstrate the absence of otoacoustic emission responses reported by Johnson et al. (2001). In the five participants who demonstrated thresholds within normal limits in the low and/or mid-frequency range, only a single isolated DPOAE in those ranges was consistent with hearing-impaired ears. None of these participants demonstrated the absence of OAE responses reported by Johnson et al. (2001). Loudness discomfort level (LDL) rating The current design of the LDL measure for this project did not allow for complete LDL measurements in the adult individuals with WS, and no data were collected for the children. The relation between hearing thresholds and LDL could not be investigated due to the small number of participants in this study. However, two out of three participants in this task assigned a rating of “too loud” at levels much lower than those expected from the general population. The pictures and descriptions for LDL measures in the current study worked well in terms of instruction comprehension. The participants in this task were able to use these pictures adequately to express their feelings about the stimuli. In a recent critical review of the most comfortable level (MCL) and LDL literature, Punch, Joseph, and Rakerd (2004) cited documentation that prior exposure to loud stimulus levels (i.e., use of a descending approach) tends to increase the MCL. This 54 literature also shows how an auditory referent influences the measures of MCL and LDL. A lack of an auditory referent in the current research design (i.e., random presentation of stimuli) may have contributed to the inconsistent responses that were observed from participant WSOO3. An ascending approach should be included in any future research design in order to provide an auditory referent and to help individuals with W8 overcome the fear of loud stimuli. The approach in the current study was flawed in that it did not include the option to continue increasing stimulus level to the point that yielded a rating of “5” In order to investigate whether individuals with W8 have a lower tolerance for loud sounds than their normal age peers, future studies should emphasize conducting LDL measures on a large group of individuals with W8, the range of stimuli used should not be predetermined, and an ascending method should be used. Hyperacusis questionnaire All of the parents of the individuals with W8 reported that their children were bothered by loud sounds in the past and were still sensitive to loud sounds at the time of data collection. All of the participants were reported to exhibit a fear response to loud sounds in childhood, and three of them (two adults and a child) have reportedly outgrown their fear to sounds. Parental reports showed that the prevalence of recurrent otitis media in the current study (two of seven individuals) was lower than the rates for individuals with W8 reported by Klein et al., 1990 (61%; ages 1 to 28 years) and Van Borsel et al., 1997 (54%; ages 1 to 45 years); however, it was close to the rate reported for normally developing children ages 1 to 17 years (26.9%, Lanphear, Byrd, Auinger, & Hall, 1997). 55 Thus the prevalence of recurrent otitis media in individuals with W8 was not as high as reported in previous studies. However, it is possible that the data in this study may not represent the general population of individuals with W8 due to the small number of the participants. Although the prevalence of recurrent otitis media was not as high as reported in previous studies, it was consistent with the tympanometric findings in the current study. Five of the participants demonstrated normal middle ear function in tympanometry during data collection. The other two participants either exhibited middle ear stiffness or were not willing to be tested in this task. Although tympanometry could not be done in these participants, parental reports revealed a history of recurrent otitis media and a fear of probe placement resulting from poor experiences with middle ear disease treatment in the past. Recurrence of otitis media could have not only contributed to conductive hearing loss, but also led to minimal-to-mild unilateral or bilateral sensorineural hearing loss (Bess et al., 1996; Hunter, Margolis, Rykken, Le, Daly, and Giebink, 1996). Two of seven participants (W8016 and W8028) in the current study were reported as having recurrent otitis media; however, their parents also reported that they were not aware of the presence of hearing loss in the past. Therefore, it was not possible to confirm that recurrent otitis media contributed to their hearing loss. The relation between recurrent otitis media and high-frequency sensorineural hearing loss in individuals with W8 has not yet been investigated. Thus future research into the cause of high-frequency hearing loss in individuals with WS should also include an investigation of the potential contribution of otitis media. 56 Evaluation procedures The current study showed that it is possible to obtain reliable behavioral thresholds from individuals with W8. All of the adult participants in the current study were able to participate in conventional pure-tone audiometry by raising a hand to respond to the pure-tone stimuli. The modified audiological assessment recommended by Kile (1996) and Dille (2003) for testing individuals with Down syndrome can be used for testing children with W8 (e.g., use of visual reinforcement to help children with W8 pay attention to the task). Another approach that would be useful would be the gradual acclimatization to the assessment procedures that was used in the data collection of 1 participant W8028. Modifications to standard procedures included letting the participant wear earphones in an environment where she felt comfortable, use of conditioned play audiometry outside the booth to help the participant practice how to respond to stimuli, use of VRA to help the participant become familiar with the testing booth, and finally, a shift from VRA to standard conditioned play audiometry in the booth. This process helped W8028 overcome the fear of earphone placement and led to reliable responses during hearing assessment. This approach would require more time than is typical in clinical settings; therefore, it will be necessary to plan ahead both for time in the clinic and in home activities. Perhaps practice at home with earphones (e.g., to listen to music) prior to hearing assessment can help individuals with W8 to get used to earphone placement. DPOAE measures should be included in the assessment of children with W8, especially when behavioral thresholds (pure-tone thresholds) cannot be obtained. Due to the frequent occurrence of otitis media in children with W8, tympanometry should be 57 also be included as a routine test in the audiological evaluation. Two child participants (W8016 and W8028) in the current study exhibited fear or dislike of the probe placement for tympanometry and DPOAE measures, or dislike of the placement of the earphones for pure-tone audiometry. Audiologists in the clinical setting or in future research should be cautious about this tendency and leave tympanometry and DPOAE measures to the end of the hearing evaluation in order to limit the impact of these fear responses on other aspects of hearing assessment. In addition, standard headphones should be used instead of insert phones during for pure-tone audiometry. Summary and conclusion Due to the small number of individuals evaluated, the data from the current study should be considered cautiously. Mild-to-moderate, sloping. sensorineural hearing loss in the high-frequency range (above 4,000 Hz) is the major finding of the current study. This finding supports ongoing reports of a high prevalence of hearing loss among children and adults with W8 and strongly challenges the historical assumption that individuals with W8 have better than normal hearing. The empirical evidence presented here demonstrates that reports of hypersensitivity to sounds do not justify a conclusion that overall hearing is intact or better than normal in individuals with WS. It is also clear that parents of individuals with W8 in the current study had low awareness of hearing impairment in their children. Bess et al. (1998) revealed the impact of sensorineural hearing impairment even at the minimal levels demonstrated by two of the three child participants in this study. An annual hearing evaluation including pure-tone audiometry, tympanometry, and DPOAE measure is strongly recommended for this population. Use of modified assessment 58 strategies can help children with W8 overcome the fear of probe or earphone placement and enable them to pay attention to the task. Once the hearing impairment is diagnosed in individuals with W8, appropriate assistance (e.g., hearing aids, classroom amplification) should be provided. Individuals with mild-to—moderate, sloping, sensorineural hearing loss in the high-frequency range (e.g., W800], W8002 and W8018) are not candidates for hearing aid fitting because hearing aids provide very limited benefit above 4,000 Hz frequency range. For these children, classroom amplification systems that increase the signal-to-noise ratio should be explored as a means to provide the teacher’s voice at a level well above the classroom noise. Children with greater degrees of hearing impairment who are hearing aid users would also be expected to benefit from such systems. Although the cause and the mechanism of hearing loss in individuals with W8 are still unknown, it is important to address fully and investigate the impact of hearing loss on these children’s language development and academic achievement. 59 - j: 1" 1' APPENDICES 60 APPENDIX A Hyperacusis Questionnaire 61 Auditory processing in W8 W8 Questionnaire NICHD R03 HD 44468-1 MSU IRB# 02-534 Child’s Name: Date of Birth: Gender: Please answer the following questions about your child’s past and present behavioral responses to loud sounds. In Column A, please answer according to the period (the child’s age) when the hyperacusis was most severe (disruptive to his/her daily life and to the life of the family). In Column B, please answer according his/her present response to loud sounds. Column A (past) Column B (present) la. Was your child presently frightened lb. Is your child presently frightened or or bothered by certain sounds? bothered by certain sounds? YES NO YES NO 2a. If your child was ever fearful of 2b. If your child “outgrew” the fear of loud sounds, at what age did this loud sounds, so what age did this begin, and was there an age when it become apparent? was the most disruptive to his/her daily life? 3a. How often had your child had ear 313- HOW often has your Child had ear infections? infections? a. _ Never a. _ Never b. __ Rarely, one or two times 13- __ Rarely. one or two times c. __ Several, over a few years C- __ Several, over a few years treated with medication. treated With medication. (1. _ Constantly, requiring d- _ Constantly, requiring medication and eventually tubes mebdication and eventually tu es 4. Had your child been diagnosed as having a permanent hearing loss? YES NO 62 IF THE ANSWERS TO QUESTIONS 1 AND 2 WERE “NO” (NO PROBLEMS WITH LOUD SOUNDS) - STOP HERE. OTHERWISE CONTINUE. Column A (past) 5a. On a scale of 1 to 5, in the right column, please rate the degree of - discomfort each sound caused your child [where 1=least discomfort and 7=greatest discomfort]. If your child has never been exposed to the sound, mark N/E (left column for “no exposure”). N/E Degree Airplane Motorcycle Loud auto muffler Garbage truck Fire engine siren Hammering a nail Power saw Electric drill Telephone ringing Firecracker Squeaking toys Dog barking Lawn mower Playground noise Loud music Vacuum cleaner Food blender Train whistle TV at normal volume level Other, please specify below 6a. How (Lid your child react to above noises? (You can choose more than one type of reaction.) a. Cover ears with hands b. Cries c. Says something like “it hurts my ear” (1. Says something like “I don’t like it” Column B (present) 5b. On a scale of 1 to 5, in the right column, please rate the degree of discomfort each sound causes your child [where 1=least discomfort and 7=greatest discomfort]. If your child has never been exposed to the sound, mark N/E (left column for “no exposure”). N/E Degree Airplane Motorcycle Loud auto muffler Garbage truck Fire engine siren Hammering a nail Power saw Electric drill Telephone ringing Firecracker Squeaking toys Dog barking Lawn mower Playground noise Loud music Vacuum cleaner Food blender Train whistle TV at normal volume level Other, please specify below 6b. How does your child react to above noises? (You can choose more than one type of reaction.) h. Cover ears with hands i. Cries j. Says something like “it hurts my ear” k. Says something like “I don’t like it” 63 Column A (past) Run away from sound Cringes Other, please specify below 7a. In general, the most important characteristics of the sounds that bothered your child M (you can check more than one if necessary). a. How loud the sound is b. A low pitched sound like an air conditioner c. A high pitched sound like a squeaky wheel (1. A sudden sound e. Reverberated sounds f. Other 8a. On a scale of 1 to 5 (where 1 is minimal discomfort and 5 is severe discomfort), how would you categorize your child’s hyperacusis {gt—its mogprominent periom his/her lite? Please circle the appropriate number. 1 2 3 4 5 minimal severe discomfort discomfort Column B (present) 1. Run away from sound m. Cringes n. Other, please specify below 7b. In general, the most important characteristics of the sounds that bother your child w (you can check more than one if necessary). a. How loud the sound is b. A low pitched sound like an air conditioner c. A high pitched sound like a squeaky wheel (1. A sudden sound e. Reverberated sounds f. Other 8b. On a scale of l to 5 (where l is minimal discomfort and 5 is severe discomfort), how would you currently categorize your child’s hyperacusis? Please circle the appropriate number. 1 2 3 4 5 ; minimal severe discomfort discomfort 9. Did you seek professional help for your child’s hyperacusis? YES NO 10. Are these any circumstance or behavior related to your child’s progress to sounds, that you think important, but have not been checked in this questionnaire? Completed by Date Relation Questionnaire was adaptedfrom Klein et a]. (1990). APPENDIX B Individual Data from Immitance Measures 65 Table 7. Individual data from immitance measures. Static admittance Ear-canal volume Gradient (mmho) (ml) Right Left Right Left Right Left W800] 0.5 0.4 2.1 1.8 0.4 0.5 W8002 1.1 1.0 0.9 1.0 0.7 0.7 WSOO3 0.3 0.4 0.8 0.9 0.5 0.5 WSOO4 0.7 0.6 1.8 1.7 0.5 0.5 W8016 NP CNE 0.4 CNE CNE CNE W8018 0.3 0.2 0.7 0.7 CNE CNE W8028 CNE CNE CNE CNE CNE CNE NP = No peak. CNE = Could not evaluate. 66 REFERENCES American National Standards Institute (ANSI, 1989). American national standard specification for audiometers. ANSI 83.6-1989. New York: ANSI. American Speech-Language-Hearing Association. (1990). Guideline for screening for hearing impairments and middle ear disorders. Asha, 32(Suppl. 2), 17-24. American Speech-Language-Hearing Association Audiology Assessment Panel 1996. (1997). Guidelines for audiologic screening. Rockville, MD: Author. Balkany, J. T., Downs, M. P., Jafek, B. W., & Krajicek, M. J. (1979). Hearing loss in Down’s syndrome: A treatable handicap more common than generally recognized. Clinical Pediatric, 18, 116-118. Bess, F. H., Dodd-Murphy, J., & Parker, R. A. (1998). Children with minimal sensorineural hearing loss: Prevalence, educational performance, and functional status. Ear & Hearing, 19(5), 339-354. Bellugi, U., Lichtenberger, L., Jones, W., & Lai, Z. (2000). The neurocognitive profile of Williams syndrome: A complex pattern of strengths and weaknesses. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12(Suppl.), 7-29. Bellugi, U., Wang, P. P., & Jemigan, T. L. (1994). Williams syndrome: A unusual neuropsychological profile. In S. H. Broman & J. Grafman (Eds.), Atypical Cognitive Deficits in Developmental Disorders: Implications for Brain Function (pp. 23-56). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. Bilger, R. C., Nuetzel, J. M., Rabinowitz, W. M. (1984). Standardization of a test of speech perception in noise. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research. 27, l, 32- 48. Chemiske, E. M., Carpenter, T. 0., Klaiman, C., Young, E., Bregman, J., Insogna, K., Schultz, R. T., Pober, B. R. (2004). Multisystem study of 20 older adults with Williams syndrome. American Journal Medical Genetics, 131A, 255-264. Davis, J. M., Elfenbein, J., Schum, R., & Bentler, R. A. (1986). Effects of mild and moderate hearing impairments on language educational, and psychosocial behavior of children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 51, 53-62. DeSilva, U., Elnitski, L., Idol, J. R., Doyle, J. L., Gan, W., Thomas J. W., Schwartz, 8., Dietrich, N. L., Beckstrom-Sternberg, 8. M., McDowell, J. C., et al. (2002). Generation and comparative analysis of approximately ~3.3 Mb of mouse genomic sequence orthologous to the region of human chromosome 7q11.23 implicated in Williams syndrome. Genomic Research, 12, 3-15. 67 Dille, M. F. (2003). Perspectives on audiological evaluation of individuals with Down syndrome. Seminar in Hearing, 24, 3, 201-210. Dirks, D. D., & Kamm, C. (1976). Psychometric functions for loudness discomfort and most comfortable loudness levels Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 19, 613-627. Dix, M. R., Hallpike, C. 8., & Hood, J. D. (1948). Observations upon the loudness recruitment phenomenon, with especial reference to the differential diagnosis of disorders of the internal ear and III nerve. Journal of Laryngology and Otology. 62, 671-686. Ewart, A. K., Morris, C. A., Atkinson, D., Jin, W., Stemes K., Spallone, P., Stock, A. D.. Leppert, M., & Keating. M. T. (1993). Hemizygosity at the elastin locus in a developmental disorder, Williams syndrome. Nature Genetics, 5, 11-16. Frank, T. (2001). High-frequency (8 to 16 kHz) reference thresholds and intrasubject threshold variability relative to ototoxicity criteria using Sennheiser HAD 200 earphone. Ear and Hearing, 22(2), 161-168. F anconi, G., Girardet, P., Schlesinger, 8., Butler, N., & Black, J.A. (1952). Chronische hypercalcaemie, kombiniert mit osteosklerose, hyperazotaemie, minderwuchs und kongenitalen missbildungen. Helvetica Paediatrica Acta, 7, 314-334. Gelfand, S. A. (2001). Essentials of A udiology, 2"d ed. (pp. 155-156, 227, 274). New York, NY: Thieme Medical Publishers, Inc. Gorga, M. P., Neely, S. T., Ohlrich, 8., Hoover, B., Redner, J., & Peters, J. (1997). From laboratory to clinic: A large scale study of distortion product otoacoustic emissions in cars with normal hearing and ears with hearing loss. Ear & Hearing. 18, 440-455. Grant, J., Karmiloff-Smith, A., Gathercole, S. A., Paterson, 8., Howlin, P., Davies, M., & wain, O. (1997). Phonological short-term memory and its relationship to language in Williams syndrome. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 2, 81-99. Greenberg, D., Wilson, W., Moore, J., & Thompson, G. (1978). Visual reinforcement audiometry (VRA) with young Down’s syndrome children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 43, 448-45 8. Hall, J. W., 111. (2000). Handbook of Otoacoustic Emissions. (pp.l28-9). San Diego, CA: Singular Publishing Group. Harrell, R. W. (2002). Puretone evaluation. In J. Katz, R. F. Burkard, & L. Medwetsky (Eds), Handbook ofClinical Audiology, 5’” ed. (pp. 71-87). Baltimore, MD: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. 68 Haskins H. (1949). A phonetically balanced test of speech discrimination for children. Unpublished master’s thesis, Northwestern University, Evanston, IL. Holmes, A. E., Niskar, A. 8., Kieszak, S. M., Rubin, C., & Brody, D. (2004). Mean and median hearing thresholds among children 6 to 19 years of age: The third national health and nutrition examination survey, 1988 to 1994, United States. Ear & Hearing, 25, 397-402. Hunter, L. L., Margolis, R. H., Rykken, J. R., Le, C. T., Daly, K. A., & Giebink, G. S. (1996). High frequency hearing loss associated with otitis media. Ear & Hearing. 17, 1-1 1. International Organization for Standards (1984). Acoustics — Threshold of hearing by air conduction as a function of age and sex for otologically normal persons. Geneva, Switzerland: 7029. Jastreboff, P. J., & Jastreboff, M. M. (2000). Tinnitus retraining therapy (TRT) as a method for treatment of tinnitus and hyperacusis patients. Journal of A merican Academy ofAudiology, 11(3), 156-161. Jerger, J ., & Jerger, S. (1980). Measurement .of hearing in adults. In Paparella, M. M., & Shumrick, D. A. (Eds). Otolaryngology, 2"“ ed. (p. 126). Philadelphia: WB Saunders. Johnson, L. B., Comeau, C. M., & Clarke, K. D. (2001). Hyperacusis in Williams syndrome. Journal of Otolaryngology, 30, 90-92. Karmiloff-Smith, A., Grant, J ., Berthoud, 1., Davies, M., Howlin, P., & wain, O. (1997). Language and Williams syndrome: How intact is “intact”? Child Development, 68, 246-262. Kawell, M. E., Kopun, J. G., & Stelmachowicz, P. G. (1988). Loudness discomfort levels in children. Ear and Hearing, 9(3), 133-136. Keiser, H., Montague, J., Wold, D., Maune, 8., Pattison, D. (1981). Hearing loss of Down syndrome. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 85 (5), 467-472. Kile, J. E. (1996). Audiologic assessment of children with Down syndrome. American Journal of Audiology, 5, 44-52. Klein, A. J., Armstrong, B. L., Greer, M. K., & Brown, F. R. (1990). Hyperacusis and otitis media in individuals with Williams syndrome. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 55, 339-344. 69 Korenberg, J., Chen, X. N., Hirota, H., Lai, Z., Bellugi, U., Burian, D., Roe, B., & Matsuoka, R. (2000). Genome structure and cognitive map of Williams syndrome. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12(Suppl.), 89-107. Lanphear, B. P., Byrd, R. 8., Auinger, P., & Hall, C. B. (1997). Increasing prevalence of recurrent otitis media among children in the United States [Electronic Version]. Pediatrics, 99(3), 6]. Lenhoff, H. M., Wang, P. P., Greenberg, F ., & Bellugi, U. (1997). Williams syndrome and the brain. Scientific American, 277, 68-73. Lowery, M. C., Morris, C. A., Ewart, A., Brothman, L. J., Zhu, X. L., Leonard, C. O. et al. (1995). Strong correlation of elastin deletions, detected by FISH, with Williams syndrome: evaluation of 235 patients. American Journal of Human Genetics, 5 7(1), 49-53. Marler, J. A., Elfenbein, J. L., Netzloff, M. L., & Liu, Y. J. (2004). Auditory system function in individuals with Williams syndrome. Poster session presented at the annual meeting of American Speech-Language-Hearing Association; Philadelphia, PA. Mendel, L. L., Danhauer, J. L., & Singh, S. (1999). Singular ’s Illustrated Dictionary of Audiology (pp. 219). San Diego: Singular Publishing Group. Merla, G., Ucla, C., Guipponi, M., & Reymond, A. (2002). Identification of additional transcripts in the Williams-Beuren syndrome critical region. Human Genetics, 110, 429-43 8. Mervis, C. B., & Klein-Tasman, B. P. (2000). Williams syndrome: Cognition, personality, and adaptive behavior. Mental Retardation and Development Disabilities Research Reviews, 6, 148-158. Mervis, C. B., & Robinson, B. F. (2000). Expressive vocabulary ability of toddlers with Williams syndrome or Down syndrome: A comparison. Developmental Neuropsychology, 17, 111-126. Mervis, C. 8., Robinson, B. F., Bertrand, J., Morris, C. A., Klein-Tasman, B. P. & Armstrong, 8. C. (2000). The Williams syndrome cognitive profile. Brain and Cognition, 44, 604-628. Mervis, C. 8., Robinson, B. F., Rowe, M. L., Becerra, A. M., & Klein-Tasman, B. P. (2003). Language abilities in individuals with Williams syndrome. International Review of Research in Mental Retardation, 27, 35-81. Morris, C. A. (2004). The latest information about inversions of the W8 region of chromosome 7. Heart to Heart, 21, 5-7 70 Morris, C., Demsey, 8., Leonard, C., Dilts, C., & Blackburn, B. (1988). Natural history of Williams syndrome: Physical characteristics. Journal of Pediatrics, 113, 318-326. Morris, C. A., & Mervis, C. B. (2000). Williams and related disorders. Annual Review of Genomics Human Genetics, 1, 461-484. Neville, H. J., Mills, D. L., & Bellugi, U. (1994). Effects of altered auditory sensitivity and age of language acquisition on the development of language-related neural systems: Preliminary studies of Williams syndrome. In S. Broman & J. Graman (Eds.), Atypical cognitive deficits in developmental disorders: implications for brain function (pp. 67-83). Hillside, NJ: Erlbaum. Nigam, A., & Samuel, P. S. (1994). Hyperacusis and Williams syndrome. Journal of Laryngology and Otology, 108, 494—496. Northern, J. L., & Downs, M. P. (2002). Hearing in Children, 5'” Ed. (p.20-23). Baltimore, MD: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. Nozza, R. J .. Bluestone, C. D., Kardatzke, D., Bachman, R. (1994). Identification of middle ear effusion by aural acoustic, admittance and otoscopy. Ear and Hearing, 15, 310-323. O’Reilly, M. F., Lacey, C., & Lancioni, G. E. (2000). Assessment of the influence of a background noise on escape-maintained problem behavior and pain behavior in a child with Williams syndrome. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 33, 511- 514. Osborne, L. R. (1999). Williams-Beuren syndrome: Unraveling the mysteries of a microdeletion disorder. Molecular Genetics Metabolism, 67, l-10. Punch, J ., Joseph, A., & Rakerd, B. (2004). Most comfortable and uncomfortable loudness levels: Six decades of research. American Journal of A udiology, 13, 144- 157. Rondal, J. A., Lambert, J. L., & Sohier, C. (1981). Elicited verbal and nonverbal imitation in Down syndrome and other mentally retarded children: A replication and extension of Berry. Language and Speech, 24, 245-254. Rossen, M. L., Jones, W., Wang, P. P., & Klima, E. S. (1995). Face processing: Remarkable sparing in Williams syndrome. Special Issue, Genetic Counseling, 6, 138-140. Ruangdaraganon, N., Tocharoentanaphol, C., Kotchabhakdi, N., & Khowsathit, P. (1999). Williams syndrome and the elastin gene in Thai patients. Journal of the Medical Association of Thailand Chotmaihet Thangphaet, 82(Suppl. 1), 174-178. 71 Singer-Harris, N. G., Bellugi, U., & Bates, E. (1997). Contrasting profiles of language development in children with Williams and Down syndromes. Developmental Neuropsychology, 12, 345-370. Stelmachowicz, P. G., Beauchaine, K. A., Kalberer, A., & Jesteadt, W. (1989). Normative thresholds in 8- to 20-kHz range as function of age. Journal of Acoustical Society of America, 66(4), 1384-1391. Tillman, T. W., & Carhart, R. (1966). An expanded test for speech discrimination utilizing CNC monosyllabic words. Northwestern University Auditory Test No. 6 (Tech. Rep. No. SAM-TDR-66-55). Brooks Air Force Base, TX: USAF School of Aerospace Medicine. wain, 0., & Yule, W. (199]). A cognitive and behavioral phenotype in Williams syndrome. Journal of Clinical Experiemental Neuropsychology, 13, 232-244. Van Borsel, J., Curfs, L. M., & Fryns, J. P. (1997). Hyperacusis in Williams syndrome: A sample survey study. Genetic Counseling, 8, 121-123. Van Naarden, K. V., Decoufle, P., & Caldwell, K. (1999). Prevalence and characteristics of children with serious hearing impairment in metropolitan Atlanta, 1991-1993. Pediatrics, 103, 3. 570-575. Volterra, V., Capirci, O., & Caselli, M. C. (2001). What atypical populations can reveal about language development: The contrast between deafness and Williams syndrome. Language and Cognitive Processes, 16, 219-239. Williams, .I. C. P., Barratt-Boyes, B. G., & Lowe. J. B. (196]). Supravalvular aortic stenosis. Circulation, 24, 1311-1318. 72