THE ROLE OF AWARENESS IN VERBAL LEARNING Thais fer the Degree of Ph. D. MICHBGAN STATE UNWERSEW Rosaréa A. Eubamfla 1965 fl'HESlS LIBRARY Michigan Staw University This is to certify that the thesis entitled THE ROLE OF AWARENESS IN VERBAL LEARNING presented by Rosaria A. Bulgarella has been accepted towards fulfillment of the requirements for _Eh_._D..._ degree in My ea 9W Major fliessor ' Date 1'38. 0-169 , r-—~--' ABSTRACT THE ROLE OF AWXRENESS IN VERBAL LEARNING by Rosaria A. Bulgarella The present study investigated the problem of verbal learning and awareness. Previous studies have indicated that learning frequently does not take place without awareness. However, these studies are illustrative of a specific area of learning in which the elicitors are weak and awareness is necessary for specification of the response. Two experiments using strong elicitors (reinforcers) were carried out. The first was a classical learning situation in which gs were classified as aware or unaware on the basis of a post- conditioning interview. The scores of the Aware and Unaware §s on a recall test were compared. No significant differences were found between these two groups. However, both the Aware and Unaware §s differed significantly from the Control Group indicating that learning took place without awareness. The second experiment was an Operant learning situation in which the‘gs were able to relieve the monotony of a task by giving a particular response. The §s were then given a rest interval containing a recorded Joke. The extent of the §s' awareness was determined by a specific, lengthy postconditioning interview. The Rosario I. Bulgarells date from the Aware §s' were eliminated from.the analysis. The findings indicated that learning did occur without ewereness since one group did show a significant rise in the correct responses end differed significantly from the Control Group. These results were predicted and interpreted in the framework of elicitation theory. ii THE ROLE 0F.IHARENESS IN VERBAL LEARNING A THESIS Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of NCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of Psychology 1965 TABLE OF CONTENTS INTRODUCTION . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . EXPERIMENT ONE . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . ... . . . . . . . Gethod . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . e . . . . . Results . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Discussion . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . EXPERIMENT TVO . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Method . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Results . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Discussion . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . GENERALDISCUSSION..................... SUMMARY . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . REFERENCES . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . ... . . . . . . ”PMDICES O O O O O O O O O- O O O O O O O O O I O O» O O O O O Page 16 16 27 30 30 35 47 50 53 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS The author wishes to express her appreciation to m. M. R. Denny, chairman of her conittee, for his theory, suggestions, and assistance in the planning of this research. Appreciation is also expressed to 11-. c. Henley, Dr. D. M. Johnson, and an. R. E. MoMichael, who gave of their time and served as members of her comittee. In addition, Mr. Bill; Fenchr deserves thanks for providing several of the jokes used in kperinent Two.- iv LIST OF TABLES TABLE Page 1. Frequency distribution of scores (number correct) on the conditioning task for the eXperimental groups. . . . 76 2. Distribution of gs in the experimental groups on the basis of postconditioning interview. . . . . . . . . . . 22 3. Frequency distribution of scores (number correct) on the recall test for the Neutral-Neutral and Control Group 0 O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O O 77 4. Frequency distribution of scores (number correct) on the recall test for aware and unaware §s in the Neutral-SpeCifiC Group. a e a e e e e e e e e e e e e e 78 5. Frequency distribution of scores (number correct) on the recall test for aware and unaware §s in the Specific~heutral Group. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 79 6. Frequency distribution of scores (number correct) on the recall test for the experimental groups. . . . . . . 80 7. Number of correct recalls on the two halves of form B bylfis in the Neutral-Neutral and Specific-Neutral Groups. 0 C O O I O O O O O O O D C O O O O O O O O O O 81 8. Distribution of gs in the experimental groups on the basis of the postrecall interview. . . . . . . . . . . . 25 9. Frequency distribution of scores (number correct) on the recall test for aware and unaware §s in the Neutral- Neutral Group based on the postrecall interview . .. . . 83 10. Frequency distribution of scores (number correct) on the recall test for aware and unaware‘§s in the Specific-Neutral Group based on the postrecall interview. 84 11. Frequency distribution of scores (number correct) on the recall test for aware and unaware §s in the Neutral- Specific Group based on the posrecall interview. . . . . 85 vi TABLE 12. 13. 14. 15. 16. Number of aware §s in each group according to which question indicated awareness. . . . . . ... . . . . . Operant rates for the Control Group and unaware §s in the expfirimental grOUPSe e e e e e e e e e e e e e 0 Summary of analysis of variance of operant rate and first five blocks of trials. . . . . . . . . . . . . Summary of analysis of variance of Operant rate and firSt bIOCk Of trials. e e e e e e e v e o e e o a a Summary of an analysis of variance on the first 30 conditioning trials. e e e e e e e e a e o e e e o 0 vii Page 35 $91 38 [40 41 LIST OF FIGURES FIGURE Page 1. Mean number of responses (75) as a function of blocks of (30) trials for the aware §s. . . . . . . . . 37 2. mean number of responses (78) as a function of blocks of (30) trials for the unaware §s. . . . . . . . . . . 39 3. Mean number of responses (75) as*a function of five blocks of (6) trials (the first block of 301 trials) fortheunaware§8eee'ee—e~eee~e-eeeeeeeeo. [52 viii INTRODUCTION The presumed role of awareness in verbal learning has changed considerably throughout the last thirty years of exPerimentation. The results of the studies first supported the S—R positions of Thorndike and Hull in which the response is considered to be mechanically (automatically) strengthened simply because it occurs in association with a reinforcer which has satisfying (or drive-reduc- ing) properties. However, with the development of more sensitive techniques for the assessment of awareness, the later findings of most experiments on verbal learning indicated that the response is not automatically strengthened. It was generally found that only aware‘fis showed an increment in performance, and this caused many investigators to abandon the 8-H positions of Thorndike and Hull. Since there appeared to be no better SAR alternative, the cognitive learning theory approach was adapted as the one best suited to predict and explain these find- ings. The investigators who follow the cognitive learning approach suggest that awareness is a necessary condition for learning and that unaware gs do not learn. In the present study, an alternative S-R mechanistic approach, elicitation theory as espoused by Danny and Adelman (1955), is used to predict and interpret the results obtained. In the framework of elicitation theory, awareness is necessary only in a particular area of learning. Learning occurs automatically in situations as specified by the theory in a subsequent section. Historical égpggtg One of the first experiments examining the problem of the relation- ship between awareness and verbal learning was that devised by Thorndike and Rock (1934). Learning without awareness was of some importance in Thorndike's theory, particularly in the realm of learning emotions, attitudes, conduct, and other similar forms of social behavior. Consequently, an experiment on verbal learning, in addition to one on motor learning, was carried out. In these studies, learning without awareness was assumed to have occurred when the rewarded responses gradually increased whereas learning with awareness was assumed when these responses increased suddenly from a moderate percentage to a high one. In the verbal experiment, E read a series of words, one at a time, to the §,who was instructed to respond by saying the first word or words which came to his mind. If the §_gave a sequential response to the word, §,called it ”right.” Hewever, if the‘§ gave a response indicating word-meaning or interpretive connections, it was called “wrong.” Thorndike and Rock found that there was a gradual rise in the number of sequential associations given by §, Since there was gradual, rather than rapid, improvement, they concluded that this was evidence fer learning without awareness. 3 Irwin, Kauffman, Prior, and Weaver (1931.) questioned Thorndike and Rock's assumption that a gradual increase in the percentage of correct responses indicated that learning took place without the § being aware of such learning. They replicated the experiment by Thorndike and Rock, except that, as a control, some of the fie were given the reason for _E_s announcements of 'right' or "wrong.“ It was found that these control gs who were told of the correct principle, and thus were aware, also showed a gradual increase in the number of sequential associations. Consequently, it was concluded that Thorndike and Bock had not given any evidence of learning without awareness. Eastman and Jarrett (1952) also used the same procedure as Thorndike and Rock. However, they used an additional criterion of awareness, that of asking the § to state the principle which he thought was correct. The fie were divided into an informed group and an un- informed group. The first group was informed of the correct principle in the beginning of the experiment, whereas the latter was not. At the and of each block of twenty words, the uninformed fie were asked if they could state the correct principle. An increase in the number of sequential associations occurred in both groups, and the a: ority of uninformed fie were. able to verbalise the principle correctly. The fie who could verbalise showed gradual but significant improvement even before verbali sation. However ,. the fie who were not able to verbalise the correct principle did not show a significant anount of learning. Consequently, this experiment did not present convincing evidence of learning without awareness. 4 Philbrick and Postman (1955) investigated the role of awareness when the correct principle was quite simple and easily applicable, rather than complex as in the previous studies. It was expected that, since the principle was more simple, learning without awareness would be more likely to occur and, with verbalisation, a rapid improvement would take place. The fie were presented with a series of stimulus words to each of which they were to respond by giving a number. The number was called correct if it was equal to the number of letters in the word minus one. The expectation that this situation would enhance learning without awareness was fulfilled in that the group which could not verbalise the correct principle did show a significant amount of learning, although performance was consistently poorer than that of the fie who eventually were successful. t study (DiVesta and Blake, 1959) utilised the same procedure as Philbrick and Postman. It was found that, regardless of whether fie are set for or are discouraged from looking for a principle, learning takes place- However, a set to look for a principle facilitated both awareness and learning. Greenapoon (1955) determined the effectiveness of two stimuli, other then 'right" and "wrong' as in the above studies. Either I'nm- hmm" or ‘huh-uh“ was given by 5 after the plural nouns for some fie or after any word not a plural noun for other fie. The fi's task was to continue saying words until 3, told him to stop. Four groups, one for each condition, were used. (lolly data from fie who were determined to be unaware by a brief, general interview were used. It was found that 5 the contingent stimulus, ”huh—uh, " had a different effect on the two groups which received it. The group which received the stimulus after non-plural responses increased their non-plural responses, whereas the group which received I'huh-uh" for plm'al responses decreased their plural responses. The contingent stimulus, "ms-ham, " on the other hand, had the effect of increasing both the plural and non-plural responses. A study similar to the one above was carried out by Sidowski (1951.). The task was the same as Greenspoon's in that the fie were asked to any words one at a time, but the reinforcing stimulus was a light blink rather than a verbal stimulus by 3. lbs results indicated that the light was an effective reinforcer. It was found that the unaware fie did increase the number of plural words when these were followed by the flash of light. Dulany (1961), also using Greenspocn's procedure, found that although the fie my not have been aware of the correct contingency between the response and the reinforcer, they may have held a correlated hypothesis. “his led them to increase their responses in such a way that it appeared they had the correct hypothesis. Some fie showed learning because they held a correlated hypothesis (saying words in a series, e.g., dianonds, rubies, and pearls), rather than the correct hypothesis (saying plural words). Delany concluded that unaware fie who showed learning may have had correlated hypotheses of which they were very much aware. Yerplank (1955) attempted to condition the content of conversation 6 by reinforcement. the verbal behavior chosen for reinforcement was the statements of opinion, and the reinforcer consisted of agreement with, or the paraphrasing of, the opinion. The meriment was carried out by a number of Es who, during an unstructured half-hour conversa- tion with an unsuspecting fi, first determined the fi's Operant level. § then followed the fi's opinions by paraphrasing or agreement. It was found that statements of epinion did increase with agreement or para- phrasing. Btinction was more difficult since some fis became angry or left to 'study' or gave some other such excuse. It was assumed that, since the fie did not indicate that the conversation was an unusual one, they were unaware that they had been taking, part in an experiment. Cohen, Relish, 1hurston, and Cohen (1951.) used a method co-only known as the Taffel procedure which consisted of giving fie 80 stimulus cards with a verb and the pronouns, "I, we, you, he, she, they,‘ on each. The fie were asked to make up sentences using one of the pro- nouns and the verb. If the fi used either pronoun of the first person, I or we, the g said 'good" in a flat unemctional voice. It was found that an increment occurred in the pronouns followed by the .E. saying 'good. " Questioning of the fie did not reveal awareness of the relation- ship between their responses and the E's behavior. mm (1955), using the some method, obtained similar results. Levin (1961) has examined the problem of whether learning without awareness is an artifact of insensitive interviewing. His method as similar to Taffel‘s sentence construction task. To test for awareness, both a brief, general interview (BI), such as in the previous studies, 7 and an extended, specific interview (El), developed by Levin, were used. Using the BI questions alone, only three out of the 60 ge. were classified as being aware. Using the additional EI questions, 16 more gp were classified as being aware. when only the BI was used to detect awareness, there was evidence for learning without awareness. However, when the E1 was used in addition to the BI, the fie who were feund to be unaware did not, as a group, exhibit learning. It is interesting to note that when the fie were aware of being reinforced for only one of the two pronouns, the fie showed learning for that pronoun alone. Levin's results threw serious doubts upon those studies which reported conditioning without awareness when only a.brief, general interview was used. Following Levin's study, experiments on awareness and verbal learning (Spielberger, Levin, and Shepard, 1962; Spielberger and DeNike, 1962; Spielberger, Berger, and Howard, 1963; DeNike and Spielberger, 1963; DeNike, 1964; and Paul, Eriksen, and Humphreys, 1962) utilised the extended interview to assess awareness. Hhen fie were determined to be unaware by the El, no significant learning occurred. The experiment by Spielberger, Levin, and Shepard (1962) examined, in addition to awareness, the attitude of the fie toward the reinforcer, Since the unaware‘fis did not exhibit learning, only the aware fie were used for the analysis of attitude upon learning. The fie were asked to choose among three alternatives on an increasing scale indicating how much they wanted the E to say "good.“ The task was similar to that of 8 Taffel (1955). The results indicated that the fin who desired the reinforcer very much showed greater learning than those who wanted it only somewhat or did not care one way or another about it. Spielberger and DeNike (1962) replicated Greenspoon's (1955) study with a few refinements. Spielberger and DeNike matched their fie more carefully as to operant rate than did Greenspoon. Using only data from unaware fie, they did not find a significant difference between these experimental fie and the control group. They concluded that Greenspoon’s positive results were artifacts of an insensitive, brief postconditioning interview and unequally matched operant rates for the experimental and control groups. In another study, utilizing Teffel's sentence construction task, Spielberger, Berger, and Howard (1963) also found no learning by unaware fie. They examined, in addition, such variables as need fer social approval (as measured by the Marlowe-Crowns Social Desirability Scale) and desire to receive the reinforcer. Again, a relationship was found between desire to receive the reinforcer, "good,“ and amount of learning for the aware fie; data from unaware gs were not used for this analysis since they showed no improvement. No relationship was observed between need for social approval and learning or desire to receive the reinforcer. The experiments by DeNike and Spielberger (1963) and DeNike (1964) add to the impressive list of studies which showed no evidence for learning without awareness. The first of these experiments considered, among other variables, the role of mediat- 9 ing cognitive states (awareness) as measured by a rather detailed interview. The fis were divided into three groups on the basis of whether they held the correct hypothesis, other hypotheses which could have augmented fis giving the response in question, or no hypotheses which affected the giving of the response. Only the‘fis having the correct hypothesis showed a significant increase in performance. DeNike (1964) attempted to determine whether the point at which awareness occurs and the point at which performance gains take place are the same. In addition to a detailed postconditioning interview, the fig were interrupted during the task (after each trial block of 25 words) and were asked to write down their thoughts about the experiment. These notes by the fis were later used to determine when awareness first took place. The results indicated that performance increments and awareness occurred on the same trial block. Paul, Eriksen, and Humphreys (1962) devised a completely novel method of studying learning and awareness. Though this experiment does not deal with verbal behavior, it is of interest because of the use of a strong elicitor. The{fis were required to sit through an experimental session in a chamber maintained at 105° 1?. They were deliberately misled as to the purpose of the study and were given I pseudo task. During this time, a puff of cool air contingent about particular physical movements, e.g., face or mouth movements, was given whenever fi gave the appropriate response. Only the aware fie showed an increase in response although almost all of the fis 10 indicated they wanted very much to receive the cool.air. However, the picture for learning without awareness is not as bleak as may appear from the above account. There have been isolated cases in which learning appears to hate occurred without awareness, and these suggest that this latter should not be considered settled. These positive.results seen to be found using strong aversive stimuli, as the elicitors (Eriksen and Kuethe, 1955; Turner and Solomon, 1962), and using responses over which the fi has no apparent control (Hefferline,, Keenan, and Earford, 1959; sorted-lino. and Keenan, 1963; Drrett, 1962). Eriksen and.Kuethe:(1955) used shock as the elicitor. £;list of words was presented to the fig several tines, and they were instructed to give the first association (word) which came to.aind.fer each of the words.. The‘fis-were misled as to the purpose of the study and were shocked.ilaediate1y after their first response to certain words. Each time they repeated the same association for which they were shocked the first tine, they were shocked again. Awareness was assessed by an intensive postconditioning interview. Both the high and the low awareness groups showed a decline in the repetition of shocked associations as compared to the nonshocked responses. There I were no significant differences in the amount and.rate of learning between the two groups. However, the high awareness group differed significantly from the low awareness group in amount of time taken to give associations to the critical and nencritical words. For the high awareness group, 11 following the first shocked trial, reaction time increased for the critical words, whereas for the low awareness group, there was no difference in reaction time for the critical and noncritical words. The increase in reaction time for the aware group corresponded with their report of deliberate suppression of the shocked responses while trying to think of new responses. The authors concluded that this difference in reaction time was an independent check upon the validity of classifying the fin into the respective groups. Turner and Solomon (1962) carried out a rather complex stub (eight experimental groups) of avoidance learning in which both the type of instruction and the amount of reflexiveness of the escape responses (respondents versus operants) was varied. nae fie were either given minimal instructions that a tone and shock would be presented or adequate instructions which also included the knowledge that they would do something to terminate or avoid the shock. The fie were interviewed at the end of the first session and at the beginning and end of the other sessions. Considering the adequately instructed fie, all groups learned to avoid and escape shock and were able to verbalise the correct avoidance response. In the minimally instructed operant groups, one group did not even show the escape response and was unable to verbelise whereas another group, similar to the shuttle box situation, was able to verbalise and learned to avoid the shock. However, in the minimally instructed respondent groups, the is were unable to verbalise either the correct escape or avoidance response and yet showed consistent escape from shock. 12 The remaining studies have in common the fact that the‘fis did not appear to be in a position to deliberately control the responses. The series hy Hefferline et al. used a covert response which was so Imall that the‘fis were unaware that they did anything. In Hefferline, Keenan, and Hartford's study (1959), fig could either escape or avoid an aversively loud noise superimposed on music by giving a small, invisible thumb-twitch. These minute responses were able to be seen hy‘fi through electromyographic amplification. ‘fis who were unaware that they could avoid or escape showed increases in the thumb-twitch from Operant level. Some of the fie who were instructed that they could avoid the aversive noise by a tiny twitch of their thumb were unable to deliberately produce such a small twitch as the unaware‘fis. The second study, (Hefferline and.Keenan, 1963), also used the small thumb-twitch as the response to be learned. fi was seated in a reclining chair and told that g was measuring ability to relax. After some time, for the establishment of operant rate, numbers appeared on a box before the fi, indicating the number of nickels fi earned. Each time 3 gave a thumb-twitch of a particular amplitude, . the number on the box would advance one. During the conditioning period, all fie increased the number of thumb-twitches over operant level. than questioned at the end of the session, no fi was able to tell how he earned the money. All as reported intense annoyance during the extinction period, indicating that earning money is a strong elicitor. Other evidence comes from studies such as Barrett's (1962) in 13 the area of medicine. A patient suffering from neuromuscular tics participated in an experiment designed to help reduce the tics. Music, which was pleasing to the g; was interrupted whenever a tic occurred. Barrett found that these tic-produced interruptions were effective in reducing the tics. Deliberate attempts by the fi’to reduce tics were not as effective as the interruptions of music. Another interesting study (Lindsley, Hobika, and Etsten, 1961), in the medical field, examined what occurred to a previously conditioned avoidance response when the fi,was anesthetized for surgery. fis who had learned an avoidance response to a loud tone prior to surgery, were found to give the response while recovering from.anesthesia even though they could not answer questions or give motor responses to verbal commands. This last group of studies presents some difficulty for those who . hold that awareness is a necessary condition for learning. The findings of these studies indicate that learning without awareness is possible, particularly under circumstances in which the elicitors are strong. The present study examines the role of awareness in verbal learning as conceived within the elicitation framework. Elicitation Ihgggz Elicitation theory'(Ibnny and Adelman, 1955; Ebony and Adelman, 1956) is presented here as an alternative S—R mechanistic position which suggests the hypotheses of the present study. It is a contiguity theory which states as an acquisition hypothesis that “the stimulus complex‘fi 14 which closely precedes in time any response elicited by any stimulus (Se) acquires the property to elicit this response" (Denny and Adelman, 1955, p. 290). unlike the drive reduction theory of Hull or Thorndike, the reinforcer serves as a form of bribery for the elicita- tion of the response to be learned rather than as a drive reducer. In other words, the response is strengthened merely because it is produced rather than because it is associated with a satisfier or reward. Learning occurs when a response is consistently elicited during a series of trials or period of time in a particular situation. Examples of consistent (strong) elicitors are the U03 in a classical condition- ing paradigm and food, water, or shock in operant learning situations. If the elicitor is a strong, consistent one and naturally elicits the response, the stimulus-response situation is automatically strengthened. In this case, the stimulus itself provides the appropriate response, and the fi,need not be aware in order to make the correct response. Awareness may occur particularly when the fi,is responding at a high level, but it is not a necessary condition for learning. In studies of animal learning, where awareness defined as verbalization cannot take place, the elicitors are necessarily quite strong, such as food which consistently elicits approach responses or shock which elicits escape responses. 0n the other hand, the stimuli, such as 'mmm-hmm' and 'good,‘ used in verbal learning studies are not strong, consistent elicitors. They do not produce, or elicit, any particular response consistently. For this reason, the positive results obtained in the early experiments 15 were unexpected. Elicitation theory predicated the opposite results from the drive reductionists who maintained that these stimuli could automatically strengthen responses since they were satisfying (showh ing approval) to the fig Elicitation purports that since these stimuli do not directly produce the response, the fi.must become aware before learning can take place (i.e., the fi’himself must provide the response in accordance with some hypothesis he holds). These weak elicitors are first used by fi,as feedback that he has made a particular response. In other words these stimuli function as knowledge of results that the fi has made an appropriate response. The _E_'s saying "mmm—hmm" or ”right" merely specifies the response that fi_should be making according to his hypothesis as to what the experiment is about. Consequently, in the area of learning with weak elicitors in which the stimulus situation does not provide the correct response, awareness is necessary for - learning. Awareness is defined here, similar to previous studies, as w the ability to verbalise (i.e., give the correct hypothesis) about the experiment. 6 In the present study, the stimuli used are presumed to be strong elicitors for most gs. It is divided into two experiments. The first of these is a classical conditioning paradigm in which, of course, the elicitor is a strong and consistent one. The second experiment is an instrumental learning situation also using a strong elicitor. The major hypotheses to be tested arse. 1) Verbal learning takes place in a classical conditioning paradigm without awareness. 2) verbal learn- ing without awareness occurs in an instrumental learning situation if the elicitor is strong. EXPERIMENT ONE Method Subjects The gs were 246 students enrolled in the introductory course in psychology at Michigan State University. Three experimental groups consisted of 82, 51, and 61 §s respectively, and a control group consisted of 52 §s. The data were obtained in five classroom situa— tions prior to termination of the teaching hour. They were collected in two sessions, a conditioning session and a recall session, with a 24 hour intersession interval. Consequently, the §s worked individually but as part of a group. Materials Used The conditioning procedure was a multiple-choice task in which a phrase, consisting of an adjective and a noun, and five alternative verbs were given (see Appendix A and B). The §pcompleted the phrase by choosing one of the five verbs. The nouns were names of occupational groups and functioned as CSs even though they followed the adjectives which served as the 0088. Each of these nouns was repeated three times with a different adjective each time, and since there were 24.noune, or occupational groups, the task consisted of 72 phrases to be completed. The adjectives, accompanying each noun, were such that one verb (the DOB) was more likely to be chosen from.the alternatives given. For example, the awkward barber, the careless barber, and the unskilled barber were placed throughout the task. These all had in common, among the 16 1’1 alternatives, the verb 'nicks.‘ The other alternatives, though plausible, did not fit the U08 as well as 'nicks" (UCR). Therefore, most gs chose "nickedt The 003 quite consistently elicited the expected 068.1 Two different forms of the conditioning task were used. Form A (see Appendix A) had the 24 nouns randomly placed three times through- out the task. For Form B (see Appendix B), the 21. nouns were divided into. two groups of 12 nouns, or 36 items, constituting the first half and the second half respectively, of Form B. These were: divided so that the number of incorrect responses was equal for each half (i.e., equal conditionability for the two halves). The eliciting value (number correct) for each noun was determined by a pilot study. The control task (see Appendix 0) consisted of 72 items, each being composed of two verbs, one of which the g was to circle as the Pfeferred. This task included the 2!. verbs from the conditioning task, each repeated three times as in the conditioning task. Thus, the control fie saw the critical verbs, or response items, as often as the merimental fie, though they did not see them paired with the meetive and occumation (“control for familiarity or recency of exposure). The postconditioning interview (see Appendix 1)) consisted of four 1 he adequacy of the conditioning task was determined by pilot studies. The adjectives, nouns, and verbs were adjusted until a vast majority of the §s chose the verb to be conditioned to the occupation. In a pilot study of 76 fie, the appropriate verbs were selected 21 to 72 times out of a possible 72. The median score was 66. Only 15 fit ”01'“ below 60 and, of these, only four scored below 52. 18 questions which allowed g to determine objectively whether fi actually knew that he was in a learning situation. Another question asked the a to explain his choice in order to determine whether § was merely guessing.. The recall test (see Appendix 3) consisted of 50 occupations, 24 of‘which were the same as in the conditioning task. The §‘s task was to add a verb-to these nouns. A;postrecall.interview (see Appendix 19 consisting of two questions was also given to sole of the fig after the recall test. This was used to determine whether the fie realised at that point that they had been in a learning situation on the previous day. Manama-as There were three main experimental groups, one given neutral instructions at the beginning of both the conditioning and the recall session (Neutral-Neutral Group), a second group which received instruoh tions specifying the task at the beginning of the conditioning session and neutral.instructions at the beginning of the recall session (Specific-Neutral Group), and a third group which received neutral instructions at the beginning of the conditioning session and instruc- tions sppcifying the responses on the recall.session (Neutral-Specific Group). V The neutral conditioning instructions given at the beginning of the first session were as follows: 'Select one alternative which best fits the beginning phrase.' Specific conditioning instructions used in 19 place of the neutral instructions at the beginning of the first session were as follows: You will note that in this task an occupation is given along with a descriptive adjective. This combination of an adjective and an occupation will strongly suggest one verb among the choices given as the best fitting one. It is your task to choose the best fitting one. Note also that the occupa- tion is repeated and the same verb. may be chosen. be neutral instructions given at the beginning of the recall session were, |'Couplete the following by adding the first verb that comes to mind.“ The specific instructions were as follows: Testerday you had to choose one verb from several which seemed to best fit the occupation. Today some of the same occupations are repeated. See if you can recall the verbs you chose yesterday. Complete the following occupations by adding the appropriate verbs. The control group was merely given the following instructions on the. first session: "Circle the letter in front of the alternative which you prefer.‘ Instructions on the: second session were the neutral ones given above for the recall session. Procedure The fin participated in a classroom situation with the cooperation of the instructors who taught the classes. The §s did not receive advance notice that an experiment was to take place. The Q merely showed up during the class period and was given a segment of the normal class hour. The E then asked the fis' cooperation and handed out the materials to the class.- 20 5:93 W. The control group received the task and instructions as shown in Appendix C. has §s in the Neutral- Neutral Group, which was made up of three small classes, received either Form A or Form B" of the conditioning task with neutral instructions. The Specific-Neutral Group and the Neutral-Specific Group were run at the same time, since the class obtained was quite large and no other class meeting within a 21. hour interval was available. All instructions were written on the first page, so the tasks for the two. groups were interlaced and handed out in such a way that part of the class received specific instructions and part received neutral instructions. The §s in the Specific- Neutral Group received either Ibrm A or Form B. be is in the [antral-Specific moup received only Form A. Along with one form of the conditioning task which included the instructions, all experimental fie received an IBM five-choice answer sheet and the postconditioning interview sheet (Appendix D). m me All groups received the test for recall of Verbs (Appendix E). Pa-t of the Neutral-Neutral (h'oup and all of the Specific-Neutral Group and Neutral-Specific Group received, in addition, the postrecall interview of two questions (Appendix F). RESULTS ConditioningITask The data for the three experimental groups on the conditioning task (number of correct responses) are presented in Table 1, Appendix G. The mean scores for the Neutral-Neutral, the Neutral-Specific, and Specific-Neutral Groups were 62.0, 65.2, and 64.2 respectively. A 2? test was used to test the differences between these experimental groups. The Neutral-Neutral and Specific-Neutral Groups did not differ significantly from each other (3’: 1.562, p ) .05), and the Neutral-Specific and Specific-Neutral Groups also did not differ (£_= .6329, p‘)».05). The differences between the Neutral-Neutral and Neutral-Specific Groups, however, was significant at the .05 level (1:, = 2.078). Table 1 also presents the data for the Aware and Unaware subgroups within the experimental groups. These subgroups within the Neutral- Neutral Group and Specific-Neutral Group did not differ significantly from each other (3’: .369, and 3,: 1.771 respectively). The Aware and Unaware subgroupswithin the Neutral-Specific Group showed a significant difference (3 = 2.087, p( .05). Postconditioning Interview Table 2 below presents a distribution of the experimental groups divided into Unaware and Aware §s on the basis of the postconditioning interview. 21 22 Mic 2. Distribution of §s in the experimental groups on the basis of postconditioning interview. Group Aware Unaware Total Neutral-N antral 36 1.6 82 Neutral—Specific 2/. 27 51 Specific-Neutral 27 34 61 Total 87 107 194 The divisions were almost equal with slightly more of the fie being categorised as unaware. The gs were: determined to be aware if they chose two of the correct multiple-choices or chose one correct alternative and gave a reasonable answer as to why they chose the alternative. If the g chose neither of the correct alternatives, the g was placed into the unware category. In cases where it was difficult to determine whether the 5 was aware or not, the § m randomly placed into either of the categories. However, in most cases, the nature of the interview allowed a clear division. A I? test was used to determine whether these were approximately the same preporticn of aware. and unaware fie in each of the experi- mental groups. The groups did not differ significantly in this "I?!“ (12 = .1381, £11: 2,. p > .90). 23 Recall Test Table 3 in Appendix G presents the recall data for the Neutral-Neutral Group, divided into Aware and Unaware gs, plus the data for the Control Group. The medians were 2.0 correct recalls for the Aware §s, 1.5 for the Unaware gp, and 0 for the Control Group. The Mann-Hhitney Q_test was used to determine whether the differencesbetween the groups were significant. As predicted, the difference between the Aware and Unaware §s within the Neutral-Neutral Group was not significant (2 ='1.02, p = .3078). The differencesbetween the Control Group and the Aware and Unaware subgroups of the Neutral-Neutral Group were both highly significant (3 = 4.90, p <..00003, and z = 4.14, p (..OOOOB), indicating learning occurred without awareness. Tables 4 and 5 of Appendix G present the recall data for the Neutral-Specific and the Specificheutral Groups again divided into Aware and Unaware subgroups. In the Neutral-Specific Group the medians were 19.5 for the Aware §s and 18.0 for the Unaware gs. In the Specific-Neutraerroup, the medians were 7.0 for the Aware §s and 4.0 for the Unaware §s. Neither of these differences between the Aware and unaware §s was significant (3 = 1.443. P = .1498, and z = 1.043, p = .2984). Table 6, Appendix G. presents the recall data for the three intact experimental groups (i.e., irrespective of awareness). The median number of verbs recalled were 2, 19, and 5 respectively for the Neutral-Neutral Group, the Neutral-Specific Group, and the 24 Specific-Neutral Group. When the differences were tested by means of the g’test, each of the distributions was found to be significantly different from the others (between Neutralpfieutral and Neutral- Specific, s = 8.63, pm< .00003; between Neutral-Neutral and Specific— Neutral, 2 = 4.19, p <_.00003; between Neutral-Specific and Specific-‘ Neutral, 2 = 7.07, p < .00003)- Lemme; As mentioned previously in the method section, Form B of the conditioning task was made up of two halves of equal conditionability.1 The number of correct recalls for the nouns ia.the first half of Fbrm B was compared to the number of correct recalls for the nouns in the second half. It was assumed that if awareness developed during the conditioning session, and thereby facilitated learning, there would be a greater number of correct recalls on the second half than on the first half. Table 7, Appendix G, presents the number of correct recalls on the first half and second half of Form B for the fie in the Neutral- Neutral and Specific-Neutral Groups. The trtest for matched groups showed no significant difference between §s'performance on the first and second halves (3,: .453, p >».60), indicating that §s did not do better on the second half. 1 The nouns of equal conditionability on the conditioning task were also found to be of equal recallability on the recall test in that gm who were given Form A showed no difference in recall to the nouns of the two halves of Farm B. 0f 40‘§s with Fbrm A, 18.§s did the same on those nouns from each half of B, 11 §s did better on the second half, and 11 §s did worse on the second half. 25 Postrecall Interview A final division of the experimental groups into Unaware and Aware fie was carried out on the basis of the postrecall interview (see Table 8 below). Only the data from initially Unaware _S_s were Table 8. Distribution of §s in the experimental groups on the basis of the postrecall interview. Group; Aware unaware Total Neutral-Neutral 24 23 47* Neutral-Specific 29 18 471". Specific-Neutral 4D 21 61 Total 93 62 155 * A class of 35 §s did not receive the postrecall in- terview and were not included above. ** Fbur §s did not fill out the postrecall interview and were not included above. recategorized. Consequently, the Aware subgroups consisted of two types of aware fist 1) those who knew that they were in a learning situation during the conditioning task in the first session and, 2) those who only realized, during the recall test in the second session, that they had been in a learning situation on the previous day. Tables 9, 10, and 11 of Appendix G show the number of correct recalls for the Aware and Unaware §s in each of the experimental groups. The medians for the Aware and Unaware §s in the Neutral- Neutral Group:were 3 and 1 correct responses respectively (Table 9). 26 The medians for the Aware and Unaware §s in the Specific-Neutral Groups were 7 and 3 respectively (Table 10). The differences in recall between the Aware and Unaware‘§s in the Neutral-Neutral Group and the Specific-Neutral Group were significant (2 = 3.15, p = .0016, and s = 2.14, p = .0324). However, the difference in recall between the Aware and Unaware §s for the Neutral-Specific Group (Table 11), the medians of which were 19 and 18 correct responses, was not significant (3 = .562, p = .5754). DISCUSSION The most important finding of this study appears to be the lack of significant differences in recall scores between the Aware and Unaware §s in all experimental groups. This finding strongly supports the major hypothesis that classical conditioning may take place without awareness. The crucial factor seems to be the use of strong elicitors. The negative results obtained by Spielberger, DeNike, Levin, and others who follow'the cognitive learning theory approach seem representative of a particular area in which the elicitors are weak. It appears that, when the response is naturally elicited, fig learn even though they are not aware that they are in a learning situation. Many Unaware fie, in this study, indicated that they believed their prejudices, or Opinions of occupations, were being measured. Others believed that the conditioning task was some sort of personality test. Yet, these §s who had no clear idea that they were in a learning situation appeared to learn as well as the Aware fie. Pbrm B afforded a completely objective check as to whether awareness, independent of its estimation during the postconditioning interview, played a role in conditioning the response. If awareness is irrelevant in classical conditioning, no improvement in perform- ance ofl§s on the second half of Form.B should have taken place. No significant improvement was found, indicating that awareness, objectively defined, did not facilitate learning. 27 28 The significant differences among the experimental groups on the recall test may be considered performance differences resulting from the sets induced by the different instructions given each group. However, as discussed above, the instructions did not differentially affect the performance of the Aware and Unaware subgroups within each of the experimental groups. This finding is consistent with the prediction that set, or awareness, should make a large differ- ence in operant learning situations where the role of the weak elicitor must be interpreted as knowledge of results, but not in classical conditioning where the apprOpriate response is clearly defined. The postrecall interview can best be considered a measure of whether the §s were using an unlimited pool of responses, or associations, for the recall task (Unaware fie) or whether they were using the pool supplied 24 hours earlier (Aware fie). If the §s were aware during recall that they were being tested for responses previously learned, they would have, most likely, given the learned response. 0n the other hand, for the §s who were not aware that they were being tested, competing responses had more of an Opportunity to interfere with learned responses. (For example, it would be much easier to give the competing response, "the barber 923g," rather than the learned response, IIthe barber gigkg.') The significant differences between the Aware and Unaware §s in the Neutral-Neutral and SpecificeNeutral Groups probably cannot be interpreted as the Aware §s showing more learning as a result of 29 awareness, since the awareness assessed by the postrecall interview did not occur during the conditioning session. It appears that the Aware §s who connected the recall test with the conditioning task suppressed competing responses whereas the Unaware §s did not. Evidence for this view comes from the lack of a significant difference between the Aware and Unaware §s in the Neutral-Specific Group who were told to give the same responses on the recall test as they did the day before. In this case, it did not matter whether the §s were aware of the learning situation, since specifying the response on the recall test eliminated stronger competing responses. The general finding of the superiority of the Neutral-Specific Group on the recall test can be attributed to the suppression of these competing responses. EXPERD’IENT TWO Method Subjects The fie were 72 fie enrolled in the introductory course in psychology at Michigan State University. They volunteered to serve for credit. They were each run individually during sessions of approximately one hour in duration. Four §s were eliminated because of apparatus failure. There was: a total of 17 fie in each ef three experimental groups and in the control group. mean on man The experiment was carried out in a mll room equipped with a desk and two chairs, one for g at the desk and one for g near one end of the desk facing the g. A screen prevented the s from seeing what the 5 was doing at the desk. A stopwatch was prominently displayed though not used as an accurate time piece. be major piece of equipment was a tape recorder ( a Uollensak 1580). The tape contained 85 short jokes or one-line gags by some of the current popular comedians, as well as a few amateurs, each separated by five seconds of canned laughter (see Appendix H, for examples of jokes). The laughter facilitated the presentation of one joke at a time. The tape recorder was started and stepped by a foot pedal which was concealed from the 5's view. However, when the foot pedal was operated, there was a distinct slit. 31' Procedure The § was seated near the desk by the tape recorder. A sheet of paper and a pencil were placed on the desk before the §. The _E_ first engaged the f, in casual conversation to establish rapport. When the § was comfortably seated, the following instructions were given: The purpose of this experiment is to measure the effect of disruptive stimuli on one’s ability to judge time. Your task is to say or give a number from care to 1000 every 3 seconds. However, you cannot repeat the same number consecutively, count, or consistently give multiples of any number, especially 10. (An example of each was given.) Before the eXperiment actually begins, we will have a practice session. I will nod.my head every three seconds to pace you, until you are able to pace yourself. Continue to say numbers every three seconds until I say stop. Hesse remember: he not count, do not repeat the same number, and do not give multiples of any number, Are there any questions? (Questions were gener answered by repeating the appropriate phrases. At this point, p displayed the stopwatch and pretended to pace _8_, making sure that he had understood the directions. If g appeared to be considerably off in his timing, § suggested that he speed up or slow down-whichever was the appropriate direction. However, 5 kept a record only of the numbers which § had given. The practice session included either two, three, or four blocks of 30 trials (i.e., a total of 60, 90, or 120 trials). This served the dual purpose of 32. determining the §¥s operant rate and increasing the §‘s boredom and fatigue with the task.1 The §s were randomly assigned to either of three groups having 60 trials (low boredom), 90 trials (medium boredom), or 120 trials (high boredom) of practice. Immediately after the practice session, p continued with.the following instructions: Now that you have had some practice, you will be given disruptive stimuli from the tape recorder to interrupt your giving of numbers and perception of time. These interruptions will be quite irreg-r ular. You are to do the same thing you did during the practice session, following the same rules. Hewever, when the tape recorder turns on, stcpv giving numbers until it turns off. Continue your task immediately after each interruption until I say, 'stop.‘ Are there any'questions? All right, begin. These instructions began the experimental session in which the tape recorder was turned on and the g was allowed to listen to one joke each time he gave a number containing the digit, "'7.‘t This digit was chosen, since it is the only two-syllable digit (besides O) and thusawas more likely to be distinguishable from the others. The jokes varied in length, but most of them.were about ten seconds long. After the firat block of 30 trials, the g asked the g to stop and read the following instructions: we are also very interested in your subjective reactions and thoughts as you participate in this experiment. So, any thus you have any kind of thought about the experiment, please stop and write your thought down on the paper before you. Number each time you stop. Continue the task again after you record your thought or reaction until I say stop. Are there any questions? 1 This task was determined to be very dull and fatiguing in a pilot study, both from gs verbal reports and from.thel§s' reluctance to smoid an aversively loud stimulus even when they were able to do so. A.few‘§s increased the response which led to the aversive stimulus and reported they did so to break the monotony of the task. 33 The experiment continued until" g gave a total of 360 numbers, including the cperant rate, or until the § had received 85 jokes. This was the total number of jokes available and, consequently, the experiment was terminated out of necessity after the supply of jokes was exhausted. The task was interrupted in three ways. The ,3 interrupted the task by giving a number which included the digit, ‘7." 'me 3 also was able to interrupt the task by saying he had a thought to write down. Lastly, the g interrupted the task at the end of each block of 30 trials and asked the § to write his thoughts down. This last interruption took place if the _8_ did not ask to stop (lining the last block of 30 trials. After the task, the postconditioning interview (see Appendix it). was given to determine whether the § had become aware. The interview is a modified version of the one used by Paul, Erikscn, and Bullphreys, (1962). If the s indicated he had become aware, g checked to make certain that 3 had written down the point at which he became amre. In the postconditioning interview, questions 1, 5, 7, 8, and 9 were used to assess awareness. mar the interview, § was asked if he had prior knowledge about the experiment. (No g indicated he had.) p then asked §'s cooperation in maintaining secrecy and gave a brief explanation of the purpose of the experiment. The procedure for the control group was the same as the 90 trial practice (medium boredom) group). The instructions did not differ. The only difference was in the way in which the jokes were given. 34 he control group randomly received six to seven jokes for each block of 30 trials during the experimental session after cperant rate was determined. The number of jokes given was based on the operant rates of the experimental groups. RESULTS Th3 Assessment 2; Awareness 0f the 51 experimental‘§s, 14 were determined to have become aware during the conditioning session. The breakdown of the number of‘§s, who first expressed awareness in response to questions 1, 5, 7, 8, or 9 of the postconditioning interview interview (Appendix I), is shown in Table 12. Table 12. Number of Aware §s in each group according to which question first indicated awareness. Question Group 1 5 7 8 9 Low Boredom. 2 1 1 Medium Boredom 2 1 High Beredom. 2 5 There was little difficulty in assessing awareness since most of these §s gave unprompted evidence of awareness to questions 1 and 5. Two additional §s gave the correct response (numbers containing the digit, '7,') to question 8. However, when asked to pick one digit out of the number, the §s did not pick '7' and conceded it could have been some other digit. They were not certain as to which was the correct response. Therefore, these'fis were classified as unaware. Seven of the 14.Aware §s were from the High Boredom Group. An additional factor minimized the difficulty of determining which §s were aware. With one exception, all Aware §s showed a decrease in 35 36 giving the digit, '7,“ as evidenced by a sharp drOp in the correct response in the block of trials after awareness.1 The performance for the Aware _S_s are shown in Figure 1. Both the Medium and High Boredom Groups showed a downward trend as more fie became stars. The low Boredom Group showed an increase toward the and because one fi gave nothing but”??? after awareness (coopera- tive rather than contrary). The slight upward trend at the end of the Medium Boredom Group, appears to support the fis‘ verbalizations that they could not suppress the response even though they tried. The dates from these Aware §s were eliminated from the main analysis. 3.2 (la—Man Brass The operant rate of each group was based on the two blocks of trials (60) of the pro-experimental practice. session. In order that the operant rates could be compared with each block of the condition- ing trials, the mean of the two blocks was used. The mean operant rates of the Control, low Boredom, Medium Boredom, and High Boredom Groups were 7.06, 5.73, 6.68, and 6.65 numbers containing the digit, ‘7," respectively. An analysis of variance (Simple-Randomized Design, Lindquist, .1953) indicated no significant differences among the four groups in mean Operant rate (F = .7916). The raw data are included in Table. 13, Appendix J. 1' than questioned, :38 gave a variety of reasons for the decline, but most said they tried to avoid the response because they felt E was trying to get them to increase it. Five of the 11. gs reported great difficulty avoiding the digit, "'7.a Even though they tried, some of the 7's slipped out anyway. A 3558. on he £03m or o w b e m acccccm homo I somehow Sago: I Eonoaom 33 4| l IQ . r enema 0r ,: N». .9. .2 (8L) SESNOdSS’H JO HEHNIIN mm 38 Berformance g; unaware fig The performance curves of the three experimental groups and the control group are presented in Figure 2. Because of the drapping out of §s who had exhausted the supply of jokes after the fifth rewarded block of trials, only the operant rates and the first five blocks are shown in Figure 2. These data were subjected to an analysis of variance (Type I Design, Lindquist, 1953). The results of this analysis are presented below in Table 14. The significant main effects Table 11.. Summary of Analysis of Variance of Operant Rate and First Five Blocks of Trials. Error line ' ' secures df MS Term. F a. Between Groups. 3 151.293 (b) 4.163. b.. Between §s in Same Group 50 36.341. c. Between Trials 5 30.308 (6) 6.178** d. Interactiont'Trials x Groups 15 7.833 (c) 1.601 s. Pooled §s 1 Trials 250 4.892 Total 323 9 Significant beyond .05 level of confidence ** Significant beyond .01 level of confidence of Groups (1? = 4.163, with 3 and 50, p. (.05) and of Trials (1‘ = 6.178, With 5 and 250 9;; p .05). .N 0.5m?" m.Honly (t_= 5.929, 41 p <(.01). (For the Control, the Low Boredom, and High Boredom Groups, the respective results are t,='1.574, p > .10; t,='.1780, p )’.803 and 3,: 1.724, p >..1o.) A trtest for independent groups was also computed between the Medium Boredom and the Control Group on the first block of trials. This difference was significant (3,: 2.209, p‘< .05), indicat— ing the expected learning effect. Since there was such a large disparity between Operant rate of emitting I'7,."and the rate in the first block of 30 trials, this first block was broken down into five blocks of six trials each. The performance curves for all groups over these trials are shown in Figure 3. These data were subjected to an analysis of variance (Type I, Lindquist, 1953). The results of the analysis are summarized below in Table 16. Table 16. Summary of an Analysis of variance on the First 30 Conditioning Trials. , Error Line Source d‘f MS Term F a. Between Groups 3 8.857 (b) 4.335** b. Between gs in Same Groups 50 2.043 0. Between Trials 4 .4700 (e) .6564 d. Interaction::Trials x Groups 12 .4375 (e) .6110 e. Pooled.§p x Trials 200 .716 Total _l_ 269 ** Significant beyond the .01 level of confidence 3453. 0 ho mmooqm m Q n N P I‘llh’ \Ila I‘lll‘ t, A 1“... 0/ \‘O ''''' $ ''''' A aoponom swam I souenom same: I Eovmhom 30H 4| I.- I4 acupeoo OI I '0 (BL) SQSNOM J0 manna MN 43 Only the main effect of Groups was significant (F = 4.335, with 3 and 50 if, P < .01). An examination of the curves shows a slight increase on the second block of six trials. However, the last few blocks show a decrease. DISCUSSION To evaluate the results properly, perhaps it would be best to examine the task in detail. The long practice session was used to augment the amount of boredom, or fatigue, with the task. (The greater the number of trials in the practice session, the greater the amount of boredom.) Therefore, there existed a partial elicit- ing state, within the organism, so that the organism tended to make a response which relieved his boredom, namely to approach the joke or rest interval, as concieved in elicitation theory. However, with each response that reduced boredom, as the background elicitor, the rest session and the joke lost their high eliciting power. In other words, the strength of the elicitor fluctuated with the building up and dissipation of boredom. Presumably, for this reason, the Medium and High Boredom Groups showed a jump from the operant rate to the first block of trials. They began to respond initially at a high rate showing that the elicitor was quite strong at that point. However, with each response that was made, the elicitor lost its effectiveness. The lack of change in performance, after the initial jump, indicates that the boredom was only enough to maintain the response performance. Confounded with the above was the‘fifs ability to interrupt the task whenever he had a “thought about the experiment." Some §s appeared to take much longer than necessary to write their thoughts 44 45 down, and one § interrupted the conditioning task as much as 40 times. These self-regulated interruptions by the §s also could hale kept them from increasing their giving of the digit”... Yet, in spite of these factors, the Medium Boredom Group showed a significant increase in the response over the Control Group. The Low Boredom Group; did not show any evidence ofnlearning. The curve of the High Boredom Group showed an upward tendency from the operant rate, though this increase was not significant either. However, almost half of these §s became aware, as might be expected if awareness follows learning, and their data were removed from the analysis. The curves for these §s showed that they gave a high number of "73:“ before awareness. As a whole, the results of this study support the hypothesis that operant learning will occur with- out awareness. The experiment by Paul, Eriksen, and Humphrcys (1962) seems? to be the only study using a strong elicitor which presents negaive results. However, the assessment of awareness was made at the end of the experiment only. No attempt was made to see whether §s improved before awareness, as in this study, where it appears that the Aware §s showed inprovement before awareness. his indicates that awareness follows learning rather than learning follows awareness. In addition, 7 out of 16 §s classified as aware in the study by Paul, et al., were classified as such by their answer to question 8 only of the postconditioning interview (similar to question 8, Appendix I, in this study). Since this is a guess 46 question, it is possible that these §s may not here been aware during the conditioning task but gave the correct answer because they had been responding at a high rate and were quite near the point of awareness. Therefore, it is not felt that the study by Paul, at sl, presents a serious contradiction to the hypothesis and findings of this study. ENERAL DISCUSSION The findings of these two eXperiments strongly suggest that learning may occur without awareness, particularly with the use of strong, consistent elicitors. The negative findings of previous studies appear to result from the use of weak elicitors. These do not naturally provide the response to be learned and necessitate mediating states (awareness) so that the § himself provides the response in accordance with a hypothesis which he holds. Meat of the laboratory studies of verbal learning involve- only weak elicitors, and the cognitive learning theory approach is adequate for the interpretation of these studies. However, the cognitive learning theory approach would be hard pressed to explain the findings of the present study. These results are consistent with elicitation theory which is a 8-H contiguity position. Elicitation theory is offered as an alternative to the Hullian type S—R theory which fails to explain past negative results in verbal learning since it holds that the stimulus-response associations are automatically strengthened because of the satisfying effect of the reinforcer. Elicitation theory, on the other hand, emphasizes the mechanistic strengthening of the response only when the stimulus situation itself provides the response naturally. Consequently, elicitation theory appears more adequate in predicting and interpreting the role of awareness in verbal learning. 47 SUMMER! A review of the literature on learning and awareness indicated that unaware §s typically do not learn. These negative results were found with weak elicitors. The purpose of this study was to examine verbal learning and awareness when the elicitors are strong. It consisted of two experiments, one being a classical conditioning situation and the other, an operant learning situation with strong elicitors. the major hypotheses are as follows: 1) Awareness is not necessary fer learning in classical conditioning.. 2) Awareness is not necessary for learning in an cperant learning situation if the elicitors are strong.‘ A.unique conditioning task was devised for the classical conditioning situation. The §s were classified as aware and unaware on the basis of a postconditioning interview. the scores of the Aware and unaware §s en a recall test were compared. as significant differences were found between these two groups. Hewever, both the Aware and unaware s; differed significantly from the Control Group indicating that learning took place without awareness. The operant learning situation was one in which the £9 could relieve the monotony of a task by giving a particular response. The 5p were given a rest interve1.containing a prerecorded joke. the task consisted of giving a number every 3 seconds, and the response to be elicited was the digit, '7." The extent of the‘fis' LI 49 awareness was determined hy a specific, lengthy postconditioning interview; The data of the Aware 5s were eliminated from the analysis.” The findings indicated that learning did occur without awareness since one group showed a significant rise in the correct response and differed significantly from.the Central Group. Both of the hypotheses were supported. It was found that learning does occur without awareness in classical conditioning and in operant learning with strong elicitors. References Barrett, B. H. Reduction in multiple tics by free operant conditionin methods. ,1. 9; Nervoug m Mental Diego” 1962, 135, $87-189. Cohen, D. D., Relish, H. I.., Thurston, J. R. , and Cohen, E. Experimental manipulation of verbal behavior. g. g2. Psychol., 1954. 47, 106-110. DeNike, D. L. The temporal relationship: between awareness “’ and performance in verbal conditioning. l. 22. Pa chol, DeNike, D. L., and Spielberger, C. D. Induced mediating states in verbal conditioning. ,1. Verb_g_l MEI—111.2 and, 122m BahaViore, 1963, 1, 339.3450 Denny, M. R., and Adelman, H. M. Elicitation Theory: I. An analysis of two typical learning situations. Psychol. £31., 1955, 62, 290—296. ’7 Danny, M. R., and Adelmsn, H. M. Elicitation theory: II. The formal theory and its application to instrumental escape and avoidance conditioning. Unpublished theoretical paper. MiOhigan State University. 1956. DiVestra, F. J ., and Blake, E. The effects of instructional sets om learning and transfer. Amer. ,1. szchol. 1959 72,, 57-67. , ’ Dulany, D. E. Hypothesis and habits in verbal “cperant conditioning." l. abnorn. oc. Pg zcholu 1961, 63, 251-263. Eriksen, G. U., and Kuethe, J. L. Avoidance conditioning without V’ awaeness. J. abnorm. so . 231212.91.» 1956, 63, 203-209. Greenspoon, J. The reinforcing effect of two spoken sounds on the frequency of two responses. her. 1. $31101. 1955 68, 409-416. ' ' Hefferline, R. F., and Keenan, B. Amplitude-induction gradient of a small-scale (covert) operant. J. m. gal. 91 behavior. , 1963, 6, 307-315. Hefferline R. F.. Keenan 3., and Harford, R. A. Escape and uoidanée conditioning,in human subjects without their observation of the response. mange,” 1959, 130, 1338-1339. Irwin, F. 3., Kaufman, K., Prior, 6., and weaver, H. E. On V ”learning without awareness of what is being learned.” 2].... 6 0‘ me, 193/» 17, 823-827e Levin, 8. M. The effects of awareness on verbal conditioning. V is me PBEChOIe, 1961, 61, 67-75. Lidsley,. O. 3., Hobeke, J. H., and Etsten, D. E. Operant behavior during anesthesia recovery: a continuous and objective method. Anesthesiology., 1961, 22, 937-946. Linden-int. 3- F. mmwawnm gag edggtion. Boston: Houghton-Mifflin 60.,“ 1953. Phul’, G. I», Eriksen, C. U.,. and Humphreys, L. G. Use of temperature stress with cool stir reinforcement for human cperant conditioning. J. 953. Psychol., 1962, 64,. 329-335. Philbrick, E. B., and Postman, L. A further analysis of “learning u. without ameness.“ Amer. ,1. B‘szchol., 1955, 68, 417-424. Ecstman, L., and Jarrett, H. An experimental analysis of |'learning without awareness." Amer. J. Fsychol., 1952, V 65. M255. """' Sidowski, J. B. Influence of awareness of reinforcement on verbal conditioning. J. 252. Echol” 1951., 1.8, 355-360. Spielberger, C. D.,. Berger, A. , and Howard, K. Conditioning of verbal behavior as a function of awareness, need for social approval, and motivation to receive reinforcement. g. abnorm. ‘ £22.» Lszcholu 1963,. 67, 24T‘246e Spielberger,. O. D., and DeNike, D. L... Operant conditioning of plural nouns: a failure to replicate the Greenspoon effect. global. _R_ep., 1962,. 11,. 355-366. Spielberger, C. D’..,;Ievin, 8.. $1., and Shepard, M. The effects of awareness and attitude toward the reinforcement on the cperant .x‘ conditioning of verbal behavior. ,1. Per!" 1962,, 30, 106-121. I/ Taffel, G. Anxiety and the conditioning of verbal behavior. do Mm 292- mg," 1955, 51,; 496-501. 59. Thorndike, E. L., and Rock, R. T. Learning without awareness,, of what is being learned or intent to learn it. J. gap. MO, 1934, 17, 1-19. Thmner, L. H., and Sblomon, R. L. Human traumatic avoidance learning: theory and experiments on the operant-respondent distinction and failure to learn. szchol. Mbnog;., 1962, 76, 1.32.. Verplank, H. 3. The control of the content of conversation: Reinforcement of statements of opinions. g. abnorm. Egg. ngchol., 1955, 51, 668-676. Appendices Lemma Conditioning Task—Porn A'. 1‘. 2. 3. 1.. 5. 9. 10. 11. 12. 13. 14. 15. 55' The thirsty carpenter (1) shovels (2) drinks* (3) leaves (4) rescues (5) drills. The careless barber (1) knows (2) sets (3) shaves (4) manipulates (5) giggg. The unluckly pilot (1) steers (2) listens (3) assists (4) delivers (5) oragheg. The unthinking dentist (1) understands (2) hurt; (3) operates (1.) files (5) drills. The hard-working laborer (1) £33223 (2) sleeps (3) sings (A) hints (5) believes. The absent-minded professor (1) buns (2) votes (3) writes (A) punishes (5) forgetg. The poor farmer (1) gtgggglgg (2) dances (3) gossips (A) 3dkes (5) prospers. The clumsy typist (1) bathes (2) suffers (3) 221.1 (A) underestimates (5) learns. The patient fisherman (1) waits (2) rushes (3) collects (A) repairs (5) navigates. The well-liked bartender (1) gripes (2) cocks (3) Judges (4) ligigag (5) swears. The cowardly soldier (1) listens (2) gaggenders (3) doubts (4) rescues (5) defends. The quiet librarian (1) gambles (2) whigperg (3) staggers (4) snickers (5) blushes. The untipped cabdriver (1) honks (2) waves (3) moves (4) gaggglgg (5) agrees. The over-weight movie star (1) fights (2) seduces (3) diet; (4) hates (5) annoys. The miserly shepkeeper (1) ggzgg (2) worries (3) loses (1.) pulls (5) curses- * The underlined verbs are the correct responses (UCH). 16. 1'7. 18. 19. 20. 21. 22. 23. 25.. 26. 27. 31.. 32. 55 The unhappy housewife (1) dusts (2) instructs (3) cocks (1.) loves (5) £533. The flattered secretary (1) Leafs (2) writes (3) fights (4) 21.12.; (5) starves. The cunning banker (1) embessle! (2) counts (3) laughs (1.) travels (5) obeys. The cocky athlete (1) help. (2) hires (3) hides (A) giggle. (5) fights. The tired maid (1) rest! (2) shouts (3) entertains (A) flirts. The cheating gambler (1) preposes (2) repairs (3) manages (1.) £1.91 (5 educates. The gay waitress (1) orders (2) giggle; (3) writes (A) slinks (5) discourages. The ill-Immored teacher (1) Jokes (2) constructs (3) trips (1.) reads (5) geoldg. The bad-tempered umpire (1) trenbles (2) zellg (3) waves (A) leafs (5) smiles. The stalled cabdriver (1) grins (3) resists (3) synpathises (1.) drives (5) $321323. The thrifty shopkeeper (1) buys (2) hinders (3) exercises (4) lies (5) gel”, The typical female movie star (1) paddles (2) diet; (3) Ph110phises (1.) dances (5) understands. . The hoarse librarian (1) m (2) questions (3) giggles (A) rages (5) arts. The agreeable bartender (1) annoys (2) listen! (3) criticises (1.) gossips (5) overcharges. _ The calls fisherman (1') deceives (2) sinks (3) mi, t; (1.) wrestles (5) swims. $151; $2? pilot (1) guides (2) rests (3) dances (1.) crageg The tipsy carpenter (1) trades (2) was (3) drinks (4) digs (5) PoundIe 33. 35. 36. 37. 38.. 39.. 1.0.. 1.1. 1.2.. 1.3.. 1.5. 46'. 47. 5:1 The «hard barber (1) understand. (2) nick; (3) succeeds (1.)" lassages (5) steals. The bad dentist (1) hurt; (2) talks (3) bums (A) comforts ('5) anesthetises. ‘ The preoccupied professor (‘1) criticises (2) forget; (a) advances (1.) chats (5) laughs. The nervous typist (1) yields (2) m (3) profits (1.) hedges (5) practices. The weary farmer (1) remembers (2) W (3) watches (1.) whistles (5) grins. The hot laborer (1) speaks: (2) trusts (3) 2229.2! (4) neddles (5) pushes. The frightened soldier (1) blackneils (2) challenges (3) investigates (4) bargins‘ (5) surrenderg. The frustrated housewife (1) scrubs (2) remembers (3) smiles (1.) gen, ('5) washes. The aggressive athlete (1) chuckles (2) specialises (3) sighs (‘3 m (.5) ”Vela The happy secretary (1) edgy; (2) telephones (3) argues (4) types (5) annoys. The desperate banker (1.) adds (2) enbessles (3) smiles: (1.) investigates (5) Jokes. The contested umpire (V1) 1.11.12. (2) pretends (3) stands (4) signals (5) snares. The lucky gambler (1) walks (2) shaves (3) fails (1.) errs The. strict teacher (1) gcoldg (2) prints (3) overlooks (1.) soothes (5) swears. Th . waitr . (523131?” ”axialwaits (2) rescues (3) giggles, Th exha tednaid (1) mm 2 Egg ('4) sack: (5) gossips. es ( ) re (3) acorns 49. 50. 51 .. 52. 53. 54. 55. 56. 5'7. 58.- 59. 63. 64. $5 The bushing librarian (1') guesses (2) assists (3) shim (1.) steals (5) 800!!- n. sympathetic bartender (1) contradicts (2) gossips (3) reasons (4) hives (5) ligeng. The hopeful fisherman (1) protects (2) gait; (3) rests (4) drives (5) cooperates. The hopeless soldier (1) gender; (2) suspects (3) cares (4) smiles (5) courts. The tough athlete (1') marries (2) cries (3) £391: (A) dances (5) synpathises. The angry umpire (1 ) pitches (2) catches (3) yell: (4) mints (5) 81.11130 11:. experienced gambler (1) gig; (2) forgets (3) falters (4) wheedles (5)” watches. The teen-age waitress (1) listens (2) snores (3) Profits (1.) simian (5) snubs. The lacy mid (1) shops (2) scrubs (3). rest. ('4) perspires (5) breathes. The neurotic teacher (1') rescues (2) advises (3) diets (A) field; (5) comforts. The sleepy pilot (1) crgghel (2) flirts (3) tries (4) units (5) tibia The unkind dentist (1) examines (2) fills (3) 911.. (1.) gm (5) wonders. The inexperienced typist (1) rides (2) boasts (3) hurries (4) 931 (5) resigns. (111.). 31131:; 1:22;: (1) protests (2) E2932! (3) questions n: er-burd d f ‘ (’5 gharges (gejmazer (1) sings (2) visits (3) m The thoughtless: professor (1) talks (2) searches (3) understands (4) mam (5) «plains. 59' 65. The unscrupulous banker (1) runs (2) calculates (3) embezzleg (4) waves (5) thinks. 66. The hostile housewife (1) ..u. (2) cherishes (3) seas. (i) forgets (5) irons. 67. The friendly secretary (1) files (2) wonders (3) sits (4) diehonors (5) smileg. 68.’ The unskilled barber (1) nick! (2) hurts (3) smokes (4) runs (5) shampooee. 69. The greedy shopkeeper (1) toils (2) sells (3) save; (4) pays struggles.- 70. The figure-conscious movie star (1) diet; (2) promises (3) sings (4) cries (5) disagrees.. 71. The typical.New Turk cabdriver (1) g;ggblgg,(2) stops (3) approves (4) pushes (5) sobs.. Appendix B Conditioning Tadk-Fbrm B 1. 3. 5. 7. 8. 9. 10. 11. 12. 13. 14. 15. 54 The well-liked bartender (1) gripes (2) cocks (3) Judges (4) ligtggg? (5) aware. The careless barber (1) knows (2) eats (3) shares (4) manipulate. (5) nickg. The flattered secretary (1) leafs (2) writes (3) fights (4) IEiLSI (5) starves. The quiet librarian (1) gambles (2) ghiggggg (3) staggers (i) snickers (5) blushes. The absent-minded professor (1) bums (2) votes (3) writes (A) punishes (5) forgetg. The unthinking dentist (1) understands (2) hurt! (3) operates (4) £11.. (5) drills. The unhappy housewife (1) dusts (2) instructs (3) cocks (4) loves (5) uses. The tired.maid (1) regtg (2) shouts (3) entertains (4) dusts (5) flirts. The over-wei ht movie star (1) fights (2) seduces (3) diet. (4) hates (5 annoys. The cowardly soldier (1) listens (2) Egggenderg (3) doubts 4) rescues (5) defends. The unlucky pilot (1) steers (2) listens (3) assists (4) delievers (5) cgggheg. The cunning banker (1) embeszleg (2) counts (3) laughs (4) travels (5) obeys. The agreeable bartender (1) annoys (2) ligtggg (3) criticises (4) gossips (5) overcharges. The exhausted maid (1) fumbles (2) regtl (3) .eern. (4) smokes (5) gossips. The awkward barber (1) understands (2) nick; (3) succeeds (4) masses (5) “Odie * The underlined verbs are the correct responses (UGR). 16. 17. 18.. 19.. Z). 21. 22.. 23. 25. 27. 28. 29. 30. 31. n 62 The desperate: banker (1) adds ('2) embezzles (3) miles (4) investigates (5) Jokes. The happy secretary (1) gig! ('2) telephones (3) argues (4) types (5) annoys. The confused pilot (1) guides (2) rests (3) dances (4) 222211216) pushes. The frightened soldier ('1) blackmails (2) challenges (3) investigates (4) barng (5) surrenderg. The hoarse librarian (1) flags“ (2) questions (3) Biggles (4) 1'8ng (5) errs. The typical female movie star ('1) paddles (2) diet; (3) philosophizes (1.) returns (5) understands. The frustrated housewife (’1) scrubs (2) remembers (‘3) smiles (4) 22s.; ('5) washes. The bad dentist (:1) hurt; (2) talks (3) bums (4) comforts (5) anesthetises. The preoccupied professor ('1) criticizes (2) forget; (3) advances (4) chats (5)1aughs. The sympathetic bartender (’1) contradicts (2) gossips (3) reasons (4) drives (5) listggg. The unscrupulous banker (1) runs (2) calculates (3) embessleg (4) waves (5) think... The unskilled barber (1) nick! ('2) hurts (3) smokes (4) runs (5) shamppoes. The sleepy pilot (1) crggheg (2) flirts (3) tries (4) waits (5) ribs. The friendly secretary (1) files (2) wonders ('3) sits (1.) dishonors (5) £2.1- The hopeless soldier (1) wonder: (2) suspects (3) cares (1.) smiles (5) courts. The figure-conscious movie star (1) diet; ('2) promises (3) since (A) cries (5) dieters... The unkind dentist (1) examines (2) fills (3) smiles (4) hurts (5) wonders. ...-.... 33. 34. 35. 41. 42. 43. 45. 46. 47. 48. a. The lazy maid (1) shops (2) scrubs (3) regtg (A) perspires (5) breathes. The bushing librarian (1) guesses (2) assists (3) ghigpggg (4) steals (5) goofs. The hostile housewife (1) sews (2) cherishes (3) nags (A) fergets (5) irons. . The thoughtless professor (1) talks (2) searches (3) under- stands (4) forggtg (5) explains. The patient fisherman (1) wait. (2) rushes (3) collects (A) repairs (5) navigates. The hardpworking laborer (1) 32232; (2) sleeps (3) sings (4) hints (5) believes. The untipped cabdriver (1) honks.(2) waves (3) moves (4) EEEEELQQ (5) agrees. The clumsy typist (1) bathes (2) suffers (3) gag! (4) underestimates (5) learns. The poor farmer (1) gtgggglgg (2) dances (3) gossips (4) Jokes (5) prospers. The badptempered umpire (1) trembles (2) 1911; (3) waves (4) loafs (5) smiles. The ill-humored teacher (1) iokes (2) constructs (3) trips (4) reads (5) goolds. The cheating gambler (1) proposes (2) repairs (3) manages (4) giag,(5) educates. The thirsty carpenter (1) shovels (2) drink; (3) leaves (4) rescues (5) drills. The miserly shopkeeper (1) ggggg (2) worries (3) loses (A) nulls (5) wees. The cocky athlete (1) helps (2) hires (3) hides (4) giggle. (5) Mg.- The gay waitress (1) orders (2) gigglgg (3) writes (A) smirks (5) discourages. 49. 50. 510 52. 53. 54. 55. 56. 57. 58. 59. 61. 62. 63. 61... a The calm fisherman (1) deoeives (2) sinks (3) §§;§£_ (‘4) wrestles (5) swims. The lucky gambler (1) walks (2) shares (3) fails (4) errs ('5) 11.92. The hot laborer (1) speaks (2) trusts (3) £22532 (4) meddles (5) pushes. The nervous waitress (1) waits (2) rescues (3) giggles (A) yells (5) socialises. The stalled cabdriver (T, grins (2) resists (3) HNNPathizes (4) drives (5) M. The aggressive athlete (1) chuckles (2) specialises (3) sighs (1.} right; (5) moves. The thrifty shopkeeper (1) we (2) hinders (3) exercises (4) lies (*5) 12293.. no nervous typist (1) yields (2) 92.: (3) profits (1.) hedges (5) practices. The tipsy carpenter (1) trades (2) saves (3) drinks (4) digs (5) pound'. The strict teacher (1) scold; (2) prints (3) ovsrlooks (A) soothes (5) swears. The contested umpire (1) zelll (2) pretends (3) stands (4) signals (5) snares. The wear: farmer (1) remembers (2) struggles (3) watches (4) whistles (5) grins.. The hopeful.fishorman (1) protests (2) ggi§g_(3) rests (A) dives (5) cooperates.. The teen-age waitress (T) listens-(2) shores (3) profits (4) $8815.93 (5) snubs. The shirtless laborer (1) protests (2) £2222! (3) questions (4) fails (5) snacks. The tough athlete (1) marries (2) cries (3) fight; (A) dances (5) sympathises. 65'. 66. 67. 68. 69.. 70. 71‘. 72. '65 The typical New York cabdriver (1) M (2) stops (3) wwoves (4) push“ (5) sobs. The greedy: shopkeeper ('1) toils (2) calls (3) meg (A) pays (,5) struggles. The dry. carpenter (1) drink; ('2) beers (3). fixes (A) crys (5, ”U8. The angry umpire ('1) pitches (’2) catches (3) zellg (4)point. (5) grins» The experienced gambler (1) 3131(2) forgets (3) falters (1.) wheedles ('5) watches. The inexperienced typist (1) rides (2) boasts (3) hurries (4) 22: (F5) resigns» The neurotic behavior teacher (’1) rescues ('2) advises (3) diets (1.) .coldg (“5) comforts. The over-burdened farmer (1) sings (‘2) visits (3) mm (1.) charges (5) jumps. Appendix G Control Task . 63 W: circle the letter in front of the alternative which you prefer. 1. (a) shovels (b) drinks 26. (a) buys (b) saves 2. (a) knows (b) nicks 2'7. (a) diets (b) understands 3. (a) assists (b) crashes 28. (a) whispers (b) questions 4. (a) hurts (b) files 29. (a) annoys (b) listens 5.. (a) sweats (b) sleeps 30. (a) waits (b) swims 6. (a) writes (13) forgets 31. (a) guides (b) crashes 7.. (a) struggles (b) PIMP”! 32.. (’a) drinks (b) pounds 8. (a) errs (b) learns 33. (a) nicks (b) steals 9.. (a) waits (b) collects 34. (a) hurts (b) hums 10. (a) listens (b) swears 35. (a) criticises (b) forgets 1‘1 ,,, (a) menders (b) doubts 36. (a) errs (b) practices 12.. (a) whispers (b) blushes 37. (a) struggles (b) whistles 13.. (a) waves (1)) grumbles 38. (a) sweats (b) saddles 11... (a) diets (b) annoys 39.. (a) bargins Cb) surrenders 15. (a) saves (b) worries 40. (s) scrubs (b) nags 16.. (a). instructs (b) range 1.1. (a) sighs (b) fights 17. on leafs (b) smiles 1.2.. (a) smiles (b) argues 18. (a) embessles (13) counts 1.3. (a) embassles (b) Jokes 19. (a) hires (b). tights 4/.- (1.) yells (b). pretends 20. C.) rests (b) flirts 1.5. (.3 fails (b) wins 21.. (a) repairs (b) wins 46. (a) scolds (b) overlooks 22. (9.) orders (b). giggles 1.7.. (a) rescues (b) giggles 23. (a) reads (1)) scolds 1.8. (a) rests (b) smokes 24. (a) trembles (b) yells 1.9. (a) guesses (b) whispers 25. (a) drives (b) grumbles 50. (.3 contradicts (b) listens 51. 52. 53. 54. 55. 56. 57. 58. 59.. 60. 61 . 63. t 65. 66.. t (a) dishonors (b) miles 68.. 69. 70.. 71.- 72. (a) waits (b) cooperates (a) surrenders (b) cares (a) marries (b) fights (a) yells (b) points (a) wins (b) wheedles ("d giggles (b) snubs (a) rests (b) breathes (a) advises (b) scolds (a) crashes (b) fibs (.1 examines (b) hurts (a) errs (b) resigns (a) protests (b) sweets (’a)’ struggles (b) Juaps (a) emches (,b) forges (a) runs ('b) embezsles (a) cherishes (b) nags (a) nicks (b) runs .(a) toils (b) saves (a) diets (b) promises, (a) grumbles (b) approves (a) drinks (b) fixes Appendix D Postconditioning Interview 1. 2. 3. mi The reason for this experiment is (circle one of the following): 8. b. c.‘ d. To find what students"reactions are to certain occupations To see whether students are consistent in their responses to certain occupations over a short period of time To teach students a particular response to a certain occupation Tb compare the favorableness or unfavorableness of one occupation to the others None of the above. Explains why you chose the alternative you did in question 1, if possible (use other side tocxif necessary): This task involves (circle one of the following): a. b. 0. d.’ Judgment Interests Prejudice Learning Personality Indicate your second choice to question 1 (on this sheet): * This response was used to assess awareness Appendix E Recall Test 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8. 9. 10. 11. 12. 13. 14. 15. 16. 17. 18. 19. 20.. 21. 23. 24- 25. The The The The The The The The The politican 26. teacherI_____J!§flgyg1______ 27. psychologist 28. barber nicks 29. bank-r M— 30. statistician 31. baker 32. advertiser 33.‘ pilot craghes 34. farmer gtggggleg n, 35. plumber 36. typist errs 37. accountant a. 38. cook 39. athlete fifigtg 40. policeman 41. bartender listens 42. nurse 43o soldier ggggendggg 44. movie star diet; 45. musician 46. dentist hurts 47. truckdriver 48. laborer ggggtg 49. lawyer 50. ‘ The storeclerk A. Librarian ..222!22££_. engineer professor Mia. Thomd__.1:29_ts____ The The mechanic painter sociologist waitress __gigglgg____ singer-x fi-hm .mn— designer housewife ___gggg_____ gardener proofreader cabdriver w fireman carpenter grinkg milkman post “was" ...sezss____ secretary AL. gambler wing electrician umpire. yell; * The correct verbs (CR) are written in. Appendix F Postrecall Interview 74 ON 3 Answer each question as fully as possible. 1.. that do you think the experiment was about? 2. During the task, did you get the idea that you were supposed to choose the verbs in any particular way? If yes, in which way or ways? Appendix G Tables of Data). From Rperimental Group 76 Table 1. Frequency Distribution of Scores (Number Correct) on the Conditioning Task for the Experimental Groups. Neutral-Neutral Neutral-Specific Specific-Neutral Score Group Group Group Aware unaware Aware Unaware Aware Unaware 4‘04 \h 0‘ COCO-4004O-sOOOMOdUdOOONNdU-fi-‘NWNUU-fi oooooooooooooooooo-s-so—so-aaowwwbmmw DOA-40400000004dOOdO-AwONOb-IomhhoxO-e g 0-.COCO-54AO-b-lOO-b—bO-bOlS-tKQNmb—Ibmmwoow 1 3 A 3 1 4 1 1 4 O O 2 2 0 O O O O O O 1 O O 0 O 0 0 0 0 O O 0 O 27' 3 dOOOOOOOOOOdOOO-lOOO-b-l—BN-I-b-bwmow Total 36 N ‘x \e 5 Mean 62.7 61.5 67.3 63.3 66.3 62.6 GROUP Total 82 51 61 77 Table 3. bequency Distribution of Scores (Number Correct) on the Recall Test for the Neutral-Neutral and Control Groups. Score Neutral-Neutral Group Control Ch‘oup Aware Unaware 24 O O O 23 O 0 0 22 0 O O 21 O O 0 20 1 O 0 19 O 0 O 18 0 0 0 17 O 0 O 16 0 0 0 15 0 0 O 14 0 1 O 13 0 0 0 12 1 1 0 11 1 O O 10 o 0 o 9 1 1 O 8 0 2 0 7 1 2 o 6 2 1 0 5 2 2 o A O 0 0 3 6 I. o 2 7 9 2 1 7 10 1o 0 7 13 40 Total 36 46 52 78 Table 4. Frequency Distribution of Scores (Number Correct) on the Recall Test for Aware and Unawareifis in the Neutral-Specific Group. Score Aware Unaware 24 2 1 23 2 2 22 3 2 20 3 5 19 3 2 18 z. 3 17 1 1 16 2 2 15 O 1 14 0 2 13 1 1 12 0 1 11 O 0 10 o 1 9 1 O 8 o o 7 0 O 6 O 1 5 o o 4. 0 0 3 0 0 2 0 1 1 0 o 0 0 o Tbtal 24 27 median 19.5 18 ‘79 Table 5. Frequency Distribution of Source (Number Correct) on the Recall Test for Aware and Unaware §s in the Specific- Neutral Group. Score Aware Unaware 24 0 1 23 O O 22 1 O 21 0 2 20 1 0 19 1 O 18 0 0 17 O 1 16 O 1 15 O 0 14. O 2 13 5 0 12 1 o 11 1 1 1o 2 o 9 O 0 8 O 2 7 2 0 6 1 2 5 3 3 4 1 3 3 1 4 2 2 4 1 1 5 O L 3 Total 27 34 Median 7 4 80 Table 6. Requency Distribution of Scores (Number correct) on the Recall Test for the Experimental Groups. Score N antral-Neutral Neutral-Specific Specifi c—Neutrel Group Group Group 21, o 3 1 23 0 I. 0 22 0 5 1 21 O 3 2 20 1 8 1 19 0 5 1 18 0 7 O 17 O 2 1 16 0 4 1 15 0 1 0 14 1 2 2 13 O 2 5 12 2, 1 1 1 1 1 0 2 10 0 1 2 9 2 1 O 8 2 0 2 7 3 0 2 6 3 1 3 5 4 O 6 4 O 0 4 3 10 0 5 2 16 1 6 1 17 O 6 O 20 O 7 Total 82 51 61 Median 2 19 5 81 Table 7. Number of Correct Recalls to the two halves of Form B by §s in the Neutral—Neutral and Specific-Neutral Groups. Subject First Half Second Half Sign 1 0 0 2 1 1 3 0 1 +1 4 1 0 -1 5 3 6 +3 6 0 2 +2 7 1 1 8 0 0 9 0 0 1o 0 1 +1 11 1 o -1 12 o 1 +1 13 1 1 1!. 1 5 +1. 15 0 O 16 0 1 +1 17 1 1 18 2 1 -1 19 0 o 20 O 0 21 1 2 +1 22 0 2 +2 23 0 1 +1 24 1 1 25 9 5 -1. 26 U 1 +1 2'7 2 3 +1 28 0 2 +2 29 8 6 -2 30 11 10 -1 31 2 1 -1 32 0 0 33 2 1 .1 34 1 1 35 2 4 +2 36 12 9 -3 3'7 O 1 +1 38 3 2 -1 39 I. 2 —2 40 0 1 +1 41 0 o 42 2 1 .1 43 6 5 -1 82. (Table 7 continued) Subject First Half Second Half Sign a o 1 +1 45 O 0 46 1 2 +1 47 7 4 -3 48 o 1 +1 49 0 1 +1 50 1 1 51 5 2 -3 52 1 2 +1 53 0 1 +1 54 0 1 +1 55 0 0 56 7 5 -2 57 1 2 4+1 58 1 1 59 2 3 +1 60 o o 61 4 2 -2 62 1 4 +3 63 5 1 -4 64 5 6 +1 65 o o 66 5 7 +2 67 o 1 +1 68 5 5 69 2 2 70 3 2 -1 a: g .a O \O N e O 83 Table ‘9. Hequency Distribution of Scores (Number Correct) on the Recall Test for Aware and Unaware §s in the Neutral- Neutral Group based on the Postrecall Interview. Score Aware Unaware 24 O 0 23 0 0 22 O O 21 O 0 20 O 0 1 9 0 O 18 O 0 17 O 0 1 6 O O 15 O 0 14 1 0 1 3 0 O 12 o 1 11 1 o 10 O O 9 1 1 8 O O 7 2 1 6 2 0 5 1 O 4 0 0 3 6 o $ 4 5 4 5 0 3 9 84» Table 10. Erequency Distribution of Scores (Number Correct) on the Recall Test for Aware and Unaware _S_s in the Specific- Neutral Group based on the Postrecall Interview. Score Aware Unaware 24 1 O 23 0 O 22 1 0 21 1 1 20 1 0 19 1 O 18 O O 17 1 0 16 1 o 15 0 O 14 1 1 13 5 0 12 1 o 11 2 O 10 2 o 9 0 O 8 1 1 7 2 O 6 1 2 2 4+ 2 3 3 3 2 2 3 3 1 2 4 O 5 2 Total 40 21 85 Table 11. Frequency Distribution of Scores (Number Correct) on the Recall Test for Aware and Unaware §s in the Neutral- Specific Group based on the Postrecall Interview.e § Unaware 21. 2 1 23 2 1 22 3 2 21 2 1 20 5 3 19 3 0 18 I. 3 17 1 1 16 3 1 15 O 1 14 O 2 13 1 1 12 1 O 11 O 0 10 0 1 9 1 0 8 O O 7 O 0 6 1 O 5 O O 4 O O 3 0 0 2 0 0 1 O 0 0 0 0 Total 29 18 Median 19 1g Appendix H Ekamples of Jokes in Experiment Two Rossi: Allen: Rossi: Allen: Rossi: Allen: Rossi: Allen: Rossi: Allen: Rossi: Allen: Rossi: Allen: Rossi: Allen: 87 “What qualifications do you need to be a spy?“ ”You've got to be a liar, a sneak, a thief, a cheat—-“ 'How did you train for this?" 'I was a used car salesman.“ “Are you married?“ ”Yeah, my wife's a princess." "A princess?” "She's built like a phone." "What would a spy do if he were attacked by a killer?“ “Some spies run, and some spies scream." "And what are you?“ ‘I'm a screaming runner." ''who's the world's greatest spy?“ “Herbie Margolis.I “Herbie Margolis! ”You see!" I never heard of him." A telephone rings First voice: “hello” Second voice: "What's the easiest way to catch a squirrel?" First voice: “Climb a tree and act like a nut.” A telephone rings First voice: "hello" Second voice: "Do you like frying?“ First voice: 'No, it hurts my arms.“ Appendix I Postconditioning Interview of hperiment Two 1’.- 2. 3. 5. 6. 7.. 8. 9. 10. 89 Would you tell me, in your own words, exactly what we are studying? For any reason, did you look forward to the. recorder turning on? Relatively how much? Here you able to concentrate on giving numbers the entire period? How well’ do you believe you did? Do. you feel you had any control over when the recorder was turned on? How? then? Would you mind telling me specifically what you thought about when you were giving the numbers? Did anything you said during the experiment have any influence upon when the recorder was turned on? that? If I were, to tell you that your saying a certain number determined when the recorder was turned on, which number would you guess it would be? Did you feel the recorder was turned on after number 7? If yes, when did you notice this? Hould you describe the fellings' you had about the following things on a five point scale from none at all to very much. a. Boredm with giving numbers b. Eagerness to hear recording c. Disappointment when recorder turned off Appendix J Operant Rate for the Control and EXperimentsl Groups 91 Table 13. Operant Rates fer the Control Group and Unaware‘fis in the Experimental Groups. Control Low Boredom. medium Boredom High Boredom [0&5 9e5 600 8‘0 10.0 4.5 4.5 8-0 9e5 705 7‘0 8'0 9.0 8.0 5.0 7.5 10.5 4.5 7.0 5.0 9.0 2.5 4.5 4.0 8.0 2.5 8.0 4.5 11.0 5.5 7.5 7.0 5.0 6.5 4.0 6.5 5.0 4.5 5.0 8.0 4.5 3.5 10.5 3.0 4.0 5.0 5.5 7.0 8.5 7.0 11.0 3.5 9.0 1.0 120.0 74.5 93.5 66.5 17 13 14. 10 7.06 5.73 6.68 6.65